50 THE FACTORS OF THE MIND central theorem in all factorial work. I have stated it as follows: " If test I is correlated with the only common factor g to the extent of rls and test 2 is correlated with the same factor to the extent of r2g, then test I and test 2 will be correlated amongst themselves to the extent of the product of those two correlations, namely,, r12 = rls, r2g: " l more briefly, the correlation between two variables is the product of their correlations with the only common factor. When we apply this theorem for purposes of prediction and the like, it is important to remember that we are dealing only with an estimation of r12 and the corresponding regression coefficient. No doubt, in theory we can show the first fraction will be unity. Similarly if the second premiss is universal. It will then follow that the third fraction must also be unity, i.e. that all the children who succeed in the test will also gain scholarships, as the syllogism concludes. (2) If (as we assume in paragraph 2) only some of the children who succeed in the test prove to be really intelligent, then we can insert the actual number, e.g. the average as deduced from our sample ; and the first fraction will be less than unity. Similarly for the second premiss. Taking the probability to be the ratio of the two frequencies (or, more accurately, the limit approached by this ratio as the number in the denominator is increased indefinitely) we arrive at the multiplicative theorem stated in para. 2, p. 47, If the argument is kept in terms of frequencies, it is equivalent to that used in the association of attributes. Thus, employing Yule's notation, let A = number who succeed in the intelligence test, JB = number who gain scholarships, C = number who are intelligent, AC = number who are intelligent among those who succeed in the test, and so on. Then, if intelli- gence is the only common factor, there will be no partial association between A and B within class C ; and BAB c = °- Accordingly, by Yule's formula, (ABC] (AC] (BC) , , .. .' r . r . , s T (2o =W'W P> 49; PP* passing, it should be noted that the < criterion for independence' within class C is really a criterion for showing that the factor responsible for the classification into C and not-C is the only common factor ; we shall recur to this below (p. 147). (3) Finally, with a slightly different line of reasoning, for " average number of (children who are also intelligent/' etc.) we may substitute " average deviation of (the same children, in intelligence)," with similar substitutions in the other fractions. We then reach the last form of the product theorem as stated in paragraph 3. 1 The Measurement of Mental Capacities•, pp. 11-12: ci also Brit* J, Psych., 1909, III, pp. 159-60; L.C.C. Report, 1917, p, 53, equation (ii). The application of the theorem is fully illustrated in each of these publications.