292 THE FACTORS OF THE MIND figures ; but he is " of opinion that his (Burt's) is a very narrow case, and that the factors considered by Burt are not typical of those in actual use " (p. 213) ; and he ends his account of the method by affirming that " it would be wrong to conclude in general that loadings and factors are reciprocal in persons and tests," since " most of what we call association or resemblance between either tests or persons is due to something over and above the very special kind of residual association " shown in " Burt's doubly centred matrix'5 (pp. 218-9). With this latter argument as worded I fully agree : for " most of the association or resemblance (i.e. most of the correlation or covariance) between either tests or persons " is due to the first or domi- nant factor (c the general factor' for tests or for persons respectively). This by definition is the factor that accounts for the greatest amount of the total variance ; and such factors are by hypothesis excluded from my ' reciprocity principle.' But, although he does not say so, the context seems to imply a belief that even the non-general (the secondary or bipolar) factors are not, as a rule, c reciprocal' or equivalent. With that I cannot agree, unless by reci- procity we mean an exact reciprocity, regardless of the inexactitude which tests * in actual use ' always entail. His chief criticisms of my argument may be summarized as follows. First, discussing my fictitious example ([101], pp. 90-4), he points out that " we could write down an infinity of possible raw matrices from which Burt's doubly centred matrix might have come " (p. 219). Thus, " to the rows of the matrix we can add any quantities we like, without altering the correlations between the tests, but making enormous changes in the correlations between persons." But, if that were done, we should be disturbing the relative difficulty of the tests: for in the rows the measurements are entered as deviations about the average for each test (or trait), and this implies that the tests are presumed to be of equal difficulty. This is a presumption common to nearly all current factorial work : (the only important exceptions are those cases in which the relative difficulty of the tests itself becomes an object of investigation, as, for example, in my investigation of the Binet-Simon scale, where the relative difficulty of the tests was studied by correlating persons). Again, he says, " we can add any quantities we like to the columns of the matrix of marks." But that would introduce differences in