CHAPTER XII GENERAL-FACTOR METHODS AND GROUP-FACTOR METHODS (a) Differences in Procedure.—The earliest attempts at factor-analysis rarely ventured to establish more than a single general factor : such a factor, operating alone, would produce a correlation matrix of rank one—a c hierarchy ' as it was termed—for which various ^criteria were proposed. Almost inevitably, so long as individual tests were used and only a dozen or so persons could be tested, the probable errors remained too high for the residuals to be significant when the general factor had been partialled out: hence no clear evidence could be found for more factors than one. But, as soon as the introduction of group-testing made it possible to diminish the sampling errors, it became manifest that the observed correlations could no longer be fitted to a strict hierarchical pattern, unless the tests were deliberately selected for this purpose, and that the discrepancies im- plied other factors besides the single c general' and the n 6 specific.' The first attempt at explicitly fitting a theoretical matrix to a set of observed correlations was, I think, that shown in Tables V and VI of my paper of 1909 ([16], pp. 161-2). The residual correla- tions, examined for further factors, were obtained in the way that has since become fairly general, namely, by simply subtracting a reconstructed hierarchy from the original correlations. The tests had been expressly chosen " to represent different types of mental process " ; and, as a result, " a small discernible tendency for sub- ordinate groups of allied tests to correlate together " was noted. With increasing reduction of the probable error there seemed no reason why the process of removing one factor to reveal another should not be repeated. Thus, as I have pointed out above, it became natural to view the empirical table, not as an unsatisfactory approximation to a single hierarchy, but as the sum of a succession 295