364 THE FACTORS OF THE MIND validity is sufficiently guaranteed by the elegance of the mathe- matical demonstration. That, indeed, is not my own interpretation of Thurstone's statements : but other readers seem to have inferred some such intention. May I, therefore, repeat that, in my view, the kind of structure to be required in a factorial matrix is not something that can be laid down a -priori once for all, but something that is necessarily determined by the selection of tests in each particular case ? In every psychological field it would be quite easy to choose a set of tests which almost certainly would, and another set which almost certainly would not, conform with a pattern like that illustrated in the table cited. Thus, Brigham's 15 tests—6 definitely verbal, 4 definitely numerical, and 5 definitely visuo-kinsesthetic—naturally lead (as we have seen) to group-factors that do not overlap—a ' simple structure ' of the step-ladder type. But we might easily include tests that overlapped in cyclic fashion, or in several varying directions at once. The other set of tests that we have examined here, the battery used in Primary Mental Abilities, shows this more irregular constitution. How far these further complications will assist or hinder factorial deductions I need not here discuss. My own opinion is that, in planning experiments beforehand, we should select our tests, so far as possible, in such a way that absence of overlap will be frequent enough to provide the necessary data for distinguishing between the factors shared by wider groups and factors common to narrower groups only. This follows from the principles on which the group- factor method is based. Even the size of the factor-variances is very much at the mercy of selection. When dealing with discontinuous groups of tests, for instance, those in Brigham's study, we could, by reducing (say) the number of verbal tests from 6 to 4 or 3, and by increasing the number of numerical tests from 4 to 6 or 7, greatly alter the relative propor- tions of the factor-variances. To insist a priori that every factor must have at least one zero, i.e. that there shall never be any factor common to all the tests, is not only an unfair predetermination of an empirical issue, but also a demand which (when we remember that factors in the actual performances must include not only abilities but also the conditions of testing and correlated errors of measure- ment or observation) is highly unlikely to be fulfilled.