440 THE FACTORS OF THE MIND but little bias in either direction doubtless find it easier to conform to the stereotyped and conventional pattern of behaviour, and so, in later years, present personalities which, to the outward observer, appear unusually correct and well-balanced.1 We may, therefore, conclude that the distribution under- lying the alleged antithetical c types' is a continuous, unimodal, bipolar distribution, closely approximating to a normal distribution. But we must add that in any par- ticular group the perfect symmetry of the normal curve is likely to be appreciably disturbed, partly by the inevitable inclusion of mild pathological cases and probably by other 1 A footnote should perhaps be added dealing with one or two criticisms that have appeared since the above was written. Dr. Jameson asks whether the conclusions drawn in my previous paper [114] are not inconsistent with my own earlier acceptance of Mendelian principles as applicable to mental as well as to physical inheritance ([22], [23]): " do not these very laws lead us to expect sharply segregated types, ' objective ' and £ subjective,' to use Hurt's own designations, * extravert' and i introvert,' * cyclothyme ? and * schizophrene' to use the more recent, and (in my view) more precise and informative terms I " Similarly, Dr. Stephenson in his latest paper on Jungs Typology argues, as we have seen (pp. 418-19 above), that the temperamental types distinguished by Kretschmer and Jung are to be regarded as * definitely limited species,' not c extreme ends of a normal frequency distribution*: " mental types," he insists, " are as distinct as the genera and species of animals in the present epcch of fauna—as cats from dogs, and buttercups from daisies." In reply to Dr. Jameson's argument it may be said that, where a character appears relatively simple in its causal origin (e.g. to take the instances he quotes from my earlier paper, sex-characteristics or red-green colour-blindness), there we may expect sharply separated types: even in colour-blindness, however, experimental tests do not reveal distinctions by any means as clear cut as is popularly supposed. But where a character appears relatively complex in its causal origin (e.g. stature or temperament) there Mendelian principles them- selves lead us to expect £ tendencies rather than clear-cut types' (cf. above, p. 246). In another paper I hope to show how the physical types popularly associated with distinctive temperamental qualities can themselves be meas- ured by factorial means, and then proved to follow a continuous and approxi- mately normal distribution. The * types' that Stephenson cites from the plant and animal world are mutually sterile ; hence there would seem to be no possibility of such types merging. But man and his * types ' are peculiar in two ways. First, man seems to have gone further than any other animal in producing markedly different varieties, which are nevertheless mutually fertile; moreover, in an intelligent and adaptable species like man a larger proportion of the more eccentric variations are able to survive and breed.