BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN OF THE flDarine Biological laboratory WOODS HOLE, MASS. Editorial Staff E. G. CONKLIN — Princeton University. JACQUES LOEB — The Rockefeller Institute for Medical Research. GEORGE T. MOORE — The Missouri Botanical Garden. T. H. MORGAN — Columbia University. W. M. WHEELER — Harvard University. E. B. WILSON — Columbia University. EMtor FRANK R. LILLIE — The University of Chicago. VOLUME XXIX. WOODS HOLE, MASS. JULY TO DECEMBER 1915 PRESS OF THE NEW ERA PRINTING COMPANY LANCASTER. PA. lirt 3S CONTENTS OF VOLUME XXIX No. i. JULY, 1915. NEWMAN, H. H. Heredity and Organic Symmetry in Armadillo Quadruplets I VAN CLEAVE, H. J. Factors Concerned in the Production of Mitosis in Organisms Displaying Cell Constancy 33 WHITNEY, DAVID D. The Production of Males and Females Controlled by Food Conditions in the English Hydatina senta. 41 CALKINS, GARY N. Microt&niella clymenellce, a New Genus and New Species of Colonial Gregarines 46 LOEB, JACQUES. The Blindness of the Cave Fauna and the Artificial Production of Blind Fish Embryos by Heterogeneous Hybridization and by Low Temperatures 50 CHURCHILL, E. P., JR. The Absorption of Fat by Freshwater Mussels 68 No. 2. AUGUST, 1915. BLAKESLEE, ALBERT F. Sexual Reactions between Hermaphro- ditic and Dioecious Mucors 87 LOEB, JACQUES. Reversible Activation and Incomplete Membrane Formation of the Unfertilized Eggs of the Sea Urchin 103 ALLEN, GEORGE D. Reversibility of the Reactions of Planaria dorotocephala to a Current of Water ill CHAPIN, CATHARINE L. A Case~qf .Hermaphroditismin Spelerpes bilineatus 129 WOODWARD, ALVALYN E. Note on the Nature and Source of " Purple X" 135 UHLENHUTH, EDUARD. Are Function and Functional Stimulus Factors in Producing and Preserving Morphological Structure? 138 No. 3. SEPTEMBER, 1915. HEILBRUN, L. V. Studies in Artificial Parthenogenesis 149 GATES, R. RUGGLES. On Successive Duplicate Mutations 204 No. 4. OCTOBER, 1915. WELLS, MORRIS M. Reactions and Resistance of Fishes in their Natural Environment to Acidity, Alkalinity and Neutrality. . 221 iii iv CONTENTS. SHUMWAY, WALDO. A Process of Temporary Chain Formation by Fronionia 258 HARMAN, MARY T. Spermatogenesis in Paratteiix 262 Xo. 5. NOVEMBER, 1915. TURNER, C. H. Notes on the Behavior of the Ant-lion with Emphasis on the Feeding Activities and Letisimulation 277 HARVEY, E. NEWTON. The Effect of Certain Organic and In- organic Substances upon. Light Production by Luminous Bacteria 308 BORING, A. M., AND POOLER, R. H. Further Notes on the Chromo- somes of the Cercopidce 312 BARROWS, WILLIAM M. The Reaction of an Orb-weaving Spider, Epeira sclopetaria Clerck, to Rhythmic Vibrations of its Web. 316 No. 6. DECEMBER, 1915. PATTERSON, J. T. Observations on the Development of Copidosoma gelechice 333 ANDREWS, E. A. Distribution of Folliculina in 1914 373 KRECKER, F. H. Phenomena of Orientation Exhibited by Ephemeridce 381 PAXMAN, DALTON G. Cell Multiplication in the Sub-cuticula of Dilepis scolacina 389 Vol. XXIX. July, 1915. No. i. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN HEREDITY AND ORGANIC SYMMETRY IN ARMA- DILLO QUADRUPLETS. I. MODES OF INHERITANCE OF BAND ANOMALIES. H. H. NEWMAN. INTRODUCTION. Since the winter of 1909, when I first secured and began the study of advanced polyembryonic fetuses of the nine-banded armadillo, I have been struck not only by the striking resem- blances among the individuals of a set of quadruplets but also by certain equally striking differences. It was early noted that well defined anomalous arrangements of scutes in the armor bands were sometimes repeated with closely similar detail in twTo or more fetuses, and were totally absent in others. Occa- sionally all four fetuses of a set showed a highly localized anomaly, but differed materially in the extent of the irregularity and in its symmetrical relations. The full significance of these conditions did not dawn upon me, however, until some years later, when a set of quadruplets was obtained in which the band anomaly in question was found to be present not only in all of the four fetuses but in the mother as well. Previous to this time only a few anomalous sets had been studied and these happened to have normal mothers, a circum- stance which led to the belief that these characters were not strictly inherited from parents but were merely predetermined early in embryonic life before the separation of the four embryos from the originally single embryonic vesicle. The finding of one unequivocal case of the direct inheritance from the mother of the anomalous character stimulated a new interest in the problem and made it necessary to collect a large amount of new i 2 H. H. NEWMAN. data. Accordingly in the winter of 1911 I journeyed from Chicago to Texas and spent about a month in the armadillo country collecting material, obtaining nearly two hundred advanced polyembryonic sets together with the armor of the mothers. A study of this material by my students and myself has brought to light a situation so interesting, but withal so intricately com- plex, that I almost despair of being able to render an intelligible account of it. Yet I cannot but be impressed with the unique- ness of the data and with the fact that it contains clews, afforded by no other material, as to methods of attacking some of the problems of the mechanics of hereditary transmission and of the development of organic symmetry. In presenting the results of the study of the heredity of band and scute anomalies I have found that the problems of heredity, and that of the symmetrical distribution of these inherited characters among the quadruplet embryos, are inseparably parts of a larger problem that has to do with the mechanics of organic symmetry. When, for example, it is found that a bilateral anomaly in a mother is inherited unilaterally in the various offspring and appears in reversed symmetrical relations in twins, we have something more than a case of simple inherit- ance. When again we find a unilateral anomaly in one pair appearing bilaterally without reversed symmetry in the opposite pair we see the problem in a more complex form. When, still further, we note that an anomaly of a single scute in the mother is inherited as such in some of the offspring and as a whole series of anomalous scutes (a band anomaly) in others, we begin to suspect that the problem is too intricate for any simple solution and that only in certain of its aspects is it likely to yield to analysis. Yet it is our duty to carry the analysis as far as our data allows. That the circumstance of polyembryony vastly complicates the already sufficiently complex problems of heredity can scarcely be denied, but it is confidently hoped that the new relations introduced by this unique type of development may throw light from a different angle upon certain phases of heredity that are now quite obscure. HEREDITY AND ORGANIC SYMMETRY IN ARMADILLO. 3 Before the reader can appreciate the significance of the data on heredity and organic symmetry, it will be necessary for him to be made familiar with the character of the material and its specific frequency and distribution. MATERIAL AND METHODS OF STUDY AND PRESENTATION. The material for this investigation consists of nearly two hundred sets of quadruplet fetuses in advanced stages, showing the definitive arrangement and number of scutes in the armor. In every case the shell of the mother was obtained and preserved so that there is on hand complete data on uniparental inheritance of armor characters. The fetuses and the respective mothers make up a collection of nearly one thousand individuals, which, is unquestionably a representative sample of the species. Counts of scutes and records of anomalies have been made- independently by at least two workers and all differences or dis- crepancies have thus been checked and corrected, so that the probability of error and personal bias have been eliminated. Since the anomalies here dealt with are rare, occurring in only about three per cent, of individuals, I have made an examina- tion of the preserved armor of 1,800 adults that formed the stock of a single dealer. This was done in order to determine the limits of diversity and the specific distribution of the anomalies., A complete record of all these anomalies has been made in the form of a pictorial diagram in Table A, I, 2 and 3. In order that the reader may understand the nature of the anomalies it will be necessary to give a brief statement of the normal relations of armor units. The primary unit of the armor is a threefold complex consisting of a bony plate, covered by a primary epidermal scale of scute, and a group of hairs that are imbedded in the bony plate and perforate the scute. For con- venience this complex is called a "scute." In young fetuses the outline of the epidermal scute is all that can be seen ; but this visible unit stands for the whole complex. These scutes or armor units of the carapace are arranged differently in the three main subdivisions of the armor, the pectoral, banded and pelvic shields. In the pectoral and pelvic regions the bony plates are arranged like tiles and form a rigid immovable shield. Naturally 4 H. H. NEWMAN. the primary scutes are arranged in more or less regular transverse rows, but these rows seldom run straight across the shield. Instead it is commoner for a row that starts single on the margin to become double for some distance where the bulge of the shell is greatest, then to be single again toward the middle. Again a row may start double laterally and be single for a short distance toward the middle. Irregularities in scute rows are commonest near the posterior border of the pectoral shield next to the banded region. In the banded region the arrangement of scutes is quite different. The bony plates are elongated antero-posteriorly to form rows of units, in general appearance somewhat like the keyboard of a piano. The posterior margin of the first band is free from and slips over the anterior margin of the second, an .arrangement that lends flexibility to the armor. The second band bears a similar relation to the third and so throughout the nine movable bands. Each bony plate is accompanied by and partially covered by an elongated, wedge-shaped primary scute. Figs, i and 3 represent two bands removed whole from the armor. The typical band, however, is composed of an even array of scutes arranged in a single row from margin to margin. Only one third of one per cent, of the 16,200 bands •examined are in any way irregular in the arrangement of the armor units. The rare exceptions, however, are of especial interest and constitute the anomalies that are the subject of the present study. These peculiar bands belong for the most part to the types, the details of which are shown in Figs. I, 3 and 5. They consist of bands that are partly single and partly double. In Fig. I is seen a very common type of bilateral anomaly in which a few scutes on each margin constitute the single part of the band, while the main central region is double. In Fig. 3 we see an equally common condition in which the double part of the band is confined to a number of scutes starting at some distance from the margins but stopping short of the middle portions of the band. Similar conditions of greater or less extent are found unilaterally as frequently as bilaterally. Now these irregular bands are in no sense abnormalities or products of injury, but are of the same nature exactly as are the irregulari- HEREDITY AND ORGANIC 'SYMMETRY IN ARMADILLO. 7 60 4 60 IZ 29 8 8 8. -y/ 3 /v // 4 9- Co 4- 12. O o 6' 6 H. H. NEWMAN. ties of scute rows in the adjacent pectoral shield, where regularity is rare and irregularity the typical condition. In about three per cent, of individuals the irregular conditions typical for the posterior part of the pectoral shield invade the adjacent parts of the banded region. That this is the correct interpreta- tion of the anomalies in the banded region is evidenced by the fact that out of 84 band anomalies recorded for this study 73 occur in the first band, which is contiguous to the pectoral shield. Of the remaining eleven cases 7 occur in band 2, I in band 3 and I in band 8, which is near the other irregular region, viz., the pelvic shield. Irregularities never occur in the middle bands of the carapace, which are farthest from the irregular pectoral and pelvic shields. Another way of interpreting the facts is to look upon the arrangement of scutes in the banded region as due to mechanical adjustments of the primordia of armor elements to flexures during embryonic development. Typically the banding confines itself to the abdominal parts of the armor but occasionally comes in a little farther forward or a little farther back and thus includes parts of the pectoral and pelvic carapace with the typical peculiarities of scute arrangements of these regions. As a result we have these anomalous cases in which the first or second band has the characteristics, doubling, etc., of the pectoral region, and the posterior bands have similarly the char- acteristics of the pelvic region. An irregularity in a given band of the banded region may involve as few as two or even one scute. Such an irregularity may consist of a single scute or two in an otherwise double band or a double scute or two in a band otherwise single. The latter situation is much the commonest of all anomalies studied. Sometimes the anomaly manifests itself by a more or less com- plete longitudinal or diagonal splitting of a single acute. Such conditions are to be dealt with separately as there is a consider- able mass of interesting data on the inheritance and distribution of these anomalous double scutes. It is impossible, however, to deal with the inheritance of anomalous band conditions with- out discovering the intimate genetic connection that exists between band and scute anomalies, for sometimes a scute anomaly in a parent reappears as a band anomaly in offspring and vice versa. HEREDITY AND ORGANIC SYMMETRY IX ARMADILLO. ~ After many experiments in tabulating the occurrence of band and scute anomalies I have adopted a simple pictorial scheme which will readily explain itself on examination of the diagram- matic Figs. 2, 4 and 7 which are simplified representations of the scute conditions shown in Figs, i, 3 and 5 respectively. The numbers of scutes in both double and single regions are repre- sented by arabic numerals and the number of the band is indi- cated sometimes, as in Table A, i, 2 and 3, by the abbreviation Bd. i or Bd. 2 just above the margin of each band, and sometimes as in table B by a single arabic numeral followed by a colon and the total number of scutes in the band. When the quadruplet fetuses and the mother are dealt with, the number of the set is indicated as A.ioi 9 or K^Oc?, indicating at the same time the sex of the litter; the bands of the mother are labeled M and those of the fetuses I., II., III. and IV. When the mother or any fetus is not tabulated the inference is that no anomaly is present in the omitted individuals. Other schemes of tabulation, such as the circular figures and those showing double scutes will be explained in the proper place. H. H. NEWMAN. SPECIFIC DISTRIBUTION AND FREQUENCY OF BAND ANOMALIES. Little need be added to the data given in Table A, 1,2, 3, which shows the anomalies found in 1,800 adult specimens. On previous occasions I have made records of over 1,000 other specimens and have failed to find any other types of diversity than those shown in this table. So it may be considered as established that we have before us in this collection an adequate representation of the diversity of band anomalies and their distribution among the various bands. Although no two ir- regular bands in unrelated individuals are just alike there are certain well-defined classes of anomaly such as the bilateral (symmetrical or asymmetrical) and the unilateral. There are types single on the margins and double in the middle; there are types double at the margin and single in the middle; and there are mixed types. Specimens 1-14 show various types of anomaly in which the bands are single at the margin with bilateral regions of doubling. Specimens 15 to 35 show unilateral expressions of the same types of anomaly. Specimens 36-42 show various mixed types, which are perhaps reversals of anomalies of the two sides of the indi- viduals. Fig. 42 is an especially interesting case of such a reversal of symmetry, for the right and left halves of the band are duplicates but are not mirror-image effects. They bear the same relation to each other as the reversed finger prints found occasionally on the right and left index fingers of human duplicate twins, as shown by Wilder. Some of the cases of exact bilateral symmetry, such as those in specimens 5, 7 and 36 are very significant, and there are all degrees of inexact bilateral duplication of anomalies ranging from specimens 12, 4, 8, n, 14 down to specimens 13, 10, 9, 6, etc. It must not be forgotten that there are no two anomalies alike in nearly three thousand specimens taken at random. When, therefore, we find, as we soon shall, exact duplicate anomalies in two or more fetuses in a set we shall not be able to explain them as coincidences. Having made clear the nature of the anomalies and their diversity and distribution in the species, we are now in a position to examine the data on the inheritance of these characters. HEREDITY AND ORGANIC SYMMETRY IN ARMADILLO. TABLE Ai. Bd. i 32, /ft 10 /e /e 3 BJ. I 3J 1 JLL 5 ?8 8 30 30 r 7 /3c/. 42- 6 22 30 /o 2,7 // 7 6 3Z 4-9 5-0 IO H. H. NEWMAN. TABLE A2. f if 16 / ZO /3d < Z 6 . r /3V./ 8 16 27 QJ.f 36 6 J.I 23 23 HEREDITY AND ORGANIC SYMMETRY IN ARMADILLO. II TABLE AS. (3d I G 10 IO 4-0 9 37 &JZ 60 6d I 6'G 18 /8 34- 33 37 8 2,0 56' 60 l GJ2 3/ /6 /O 39 39 TT 8 27 AZ. Z7 ~ 12 H. H. NEWMAN. THE INHERITANCE AND DISTRIBUTION OF BAND ANOMALIES. After a study of over 150 of the most advanced sets of quad- ruplets in my collection I am convinced that both band and scute anomalies are strongly inherited. In every case in which a mother exhibits a band or a scute anomaly, a related anomaly is found in one or more of the offspring. If the character were not inherited as a dominant, one would expect some exceptions to this rule, but none have been found. When, therefore, we find an equal number of offspring of normal mothers exhibiting anomalies of the same sort we are justified in concluding that the characters represent a heritage from the unknown fathers. This assumption is farther justified by the finding that the char- acters in question are neither sex-limited nor sex-linked. For our purposes, then, the data here published are adequate in that a study of uniparental inheritance reveals fully the modes of inheritance that obtain for band and scute anomalies. What- ever genetic relations are found to hold between mothers and offspring would doubtless hold for fathers and offspring. The only unfortunate complication that is encountered is in connec- tion with a small per cent, of cases in which both fathers and mothers possess anomalies. A few sets of fetuses are obviously of this dual anomalous parentage, and we can, by knowing the maternal anomaly, make a well-founded conjecture as to the probable nature of the anomaly in the unknown father. Whether or not I am justified in assuming that the study of maternal inheritance reveals the essential facts concerning the inheritance of the characters in question, can be settled only by breeding and, as has been pointed out in extenso in an' earlier paper (Newman, '13), breeding experiments with the armadillo are at present totally impracticable. Consequently, we are forced to rely upon a study of inheritance from one parent, the mother. The nature of the anomalies is such that I have been unable to devise any really convenient method for tabulating the facts that must be known about them. It seems necessary to consider each case of inheritance separately, and this may be done without undue prolixity because the cases are not numerous. The pictorial method appears to be well adapted for the data, but it HEREDITY AND ORGANIC SYMMETRY IX ARMADILLO. 13 would be too tedious a task to draw each anomaly in detail after the fashion of Figs. I and 3. Instead I shall use the diagrammatic form seen in Figs. 2 and 4, which give all the necessary informa- tion. The best method I have been able to devise for showing the symmetrical or asymmetrical relations of the anomalies and their distribution among the quadruplet fetuses of a set is that shown in Figs. 9-16, in which the anomalous bands are placed as though within the embryonic vesicle. The reader must remember that the structures studied are integumentary units, that the embryonic vesicle is so inverted that the ectoderm forms the inner lining and the endoderm is on the exterior. The diagram (Fig. 8) shows the relative positions of the band anomalies of the fetuses in set 87 (Fig. 9). Here we have an equatorial cross section of an advanced vesicle with the body of each fetus severed at the first armor band where the anomaly occurs. The ventral surfaces face outward and are attached by their respective umbilical cords to the placental portion of the vesicle. The dorsal aspects are turned inward. Right and left sides of each fetus are indicated. The reader must imagine himself in the center of the vesicle facing outward toward each fetus. In order to simplify this type of diagram, I have found it best to ignore all structures but the band in question and to represent the latter as though straightened out against the periphery. The reader will understand the adopted form of diagram after a comparison of Fig. 8, which represents nearly the actual relations present in set 87, and Fig. 9, which is a diagram of the same set showing only the bands that are of interest in this study. In both figures the solid quadrant line divides the twins I. and II. from twins III. and IV. and the dotted line separates twin individuals, I. from II. and III. from IV. In the actual vesicle these quadrant lines are occupied by amniotic partitions that hermetically isolate each fetus from the others. When the anomaly is found in the mother also the material condition is indicated in a straight band diagram beside or beneath the circle showing the offspring. When any individual shows an anomaly in more than one band, the more anterior anomaly is. shown in the circle and the more posterior anomaly in a concentric sector outside of the circle, as in Fig. 9. 14 H. H. NEWMAN. Of the twenty-six sets of fetuses one or more individuals of each of which show band anomalies, twelve came from mothers that also showed either band or acute anomalies, and fourteen came from mothers that showed no anomaly. Presumably the anomaly has been inherited from the father in these fourteen cases. Of the twenty-six sets showing band anomalies exactly half are male and half female, which demonstrates that the characters dealt with have no sex-limitation or sex-linkage. Males and females inherit equally strongly from the mothers, for of the twelve sets of offspring in which the mother also shows an anomaly, five are of the female and seven of the male sex. In the case of four sets with anomalous mothers and similarly in four sets with normal mothers, I shall follow the plan of placing a circular diagram and the verbal description and analyses close together in the text. The remaining sets are tabulated in somewhat more compact form and placed at the end of the paper. Those in Table B are the eight remaining sets that have the anomaly in both mother and one or more offspring; those in Table C are the remaining ten sets with band anomaly in one or more fetuses but with normal mothers. Each set is placed in a separate block and labeled at the upper left-hand corner, with the number of the set and the sex of the young, as C 29 cf . The number of the band showing the anomaly is indicated in two ways, as bd. I or bd. 2 placed next to or on the band, or by a number placed on the band followed by a colon and the total number of scutes in the band, as 7: 65, meaning band 7 having a total of 65 scutes. In bands more or less broken by doubling the number of scutes in each part is indicated. In the case of short series of double scutes or one double scute, the number placed on the element, together with the arrow, indicates the numerical position of the element from either margin, or from the middle, which is indicated by a dotted line. Set K. 87, 9 (Figs. 8 and Q). This set shows more certainly than any other the direct inheritance of a material band anomaly by all of the offspring. In the mother the anomaly consists of a unilateral local doubling HEREDITY AND ORGANIC_SYMMETRY IX ARMADILLO. 15 8 3 o H X PI 7) 1 6 H. H. NEWMAN. of band I, confined to the left half of the band and almost identical with the left half of the band pictured in Fig. 3. The band begins on the left lateral margin with six single scutes, is then double for 14-13 scutes and single for the remainder of the series. No other anomalies are exhibited by the mother. Fetuses I., II. and IV. show similar anomalies of band I, but they are unequally bilateral in their expression (like Fig. 3), all three showing more scutes double on the left than on the right. Fetuses I. and II. are strikingly similar in the distribution of single and double scutes and show no reversal of symmetry. Fetus IV. is the most nearly bilaterally symmetrical in the dis- position of its scutes. Fetus III. is unilateral like the mother, but shows a reversal of symmetry in that the doubling is confined to the right side. The symmetry of fetus III. is also a reversal of that of fetuses I., II. and IV., which show a preponderance of doubling on the left side. In addition to the anomalies referred to, fetuses I. and II. show scute anomalies of bands 4 and 5 respectively that do not appear to be traceable to the mother and have presumably come from the father. This is one of the rare sets in which it is probable that anomalies came from both sides of the family. The double scutes of I. and II. are in a reversed symmetry to each other, being near the right in fetus I. and near the left in fetus II. Set K.JO, 9 (Fig. 10} . This set is of importance in that it deals with short rows of double scutes ranging from 2 to 7 elements and serves to empha- size the genetic connection between plate and scute anomalies. The mother has in band I in the positions 7 and 8 from the left two adjacent double scutes forming a band anomaly of minimal size but of the same character as the short double regions of the band pictured in Fig. 5. If only one scute had been double-tiered, we would have called it a "scute" anomaly instead of a "band" anomaly. There is also in the mother an independent double scute situated 10 places to the left of the middle of band 2. Fetus I. has a double scute in band I in exactly the position of the first of the two double scutes of the mother. This seems HEREDITY AND ORGANIC SYMMETRY IN ARMADILLO. 17 to indicate that a double scute and an incipient double band are genetically equivalent. Fetus II. has in the exact middle of band I a short series of three double scutes like the two which are near the left margin of the mother; and in the same band, midway between the middle and left band margin, one double scute. In band 6 of the same fetus there occurs at a point three scutes to the left of the middle another double scute like that in band i. Note that all asymmetrical anomalies are on the left side as in the mother. 54 MOTH e ' Bindl 7?. 10 Fetus III. has one double scute in the exact middle of band I, or in a position identical with the short double band in fetus II. Fetus IV. has a double region of 7-7 scutes in the exact middle of band I. This doubling is simply a more pronounced case of the doubling shown in the mother as in fetuses II. and III. The points of interest illustrated by this set are: I. In mother and offspring the unilateral symmetry is the same throughout except that there is a tendency for the anomaly to shift to the center of the band. This shifting of lateral anomalies to the center is a peculiar type of symmetry reversal in which the central and lateral portions of each half band become i8 H. H. NEWMAN. reversed. That this is the correct interpretation of the situation is amply shown by many cases shown subsequently. 2. There occur several examples of the reduplication of a single inherited unit, sometimes involving a single band, sometimes two or more. Doubtless the anomalies of band I and band 2 in the mother represent a merely reversed reduplication of the same inheritance unit. Similarly in fetus II. the series of three double scutes in the middle of band I, the double scute near the middle of band 6 and the scute in the middle of the left half of band I are varied manifestations of the same anomaly. 3. There is more extensive doubling in the two primary bud individuals II. and IV. than in the two secondary bud individuals I. and HI. S-3-/0 II Set K. 4, «- 8 g: o ' '8 y' 9 1 1 r? 9 o L > 9 1 7 y 9 ^ TT ( 6 / SJ ' _LL ^ K P^ > Bd 1 5. 6 _,< I-1- 32 p 2 ^6 ± ll <- -'g 5^(58 P 1 :64 ^7 F 3 .63 7 ^ r.s 3 tn f .65 V 9-^ . 14 cf The mother has an incipient double band of two scutes in positions 5 and 6 to the left of the middle of band 5. Fetus I. has a double scute in position 4 to the right of the middle of band 8. This shows reversed symmetry and a shifting down the primary axis of the genetic unit inherited from the mother. 24 H. H. NEWMAN. Fetus II. has a double scute in position 2 from the left-hand margin of band 3. This is evidently a reversal of the conditions seen in the mother and is also a complex reversal, with shifted position on the primary axis, of the condition seen in its twin partner, fetus I. Fetus IV. has a double scute in band 6, situated with refer- ence to the middle of the left half band in about the same relation as is the peculiarity of the mother with reference to the middle of the whole band. In other words it is 5 scutes to the left of the middle of the left half of band 6, while that of the mother is situated 5 scutes to the left of the middle of band 5. Considering the rarity of anomalies of any sort in the bands 3, 4, 5 and 6, there can be no doubt of the genetic identity of these elements. Granting this the interpretation offered is not forced. Set K. 75, 9 (Table B 2}. Here the mother has two double scutes, one near the middle of the right half of band I and another to the right of the middle of band 2. Fetus I. has a double scute in band 2 identical in position with one of those of the mother. Fetus IV. has in band 8, in a position with reference to the middle identical with one of those of the mother and that of fetus I., an incipient band doubling of 2 scutes. This is another example of the shifting of an anterior element to a position further down the primary axis. It also emphasizes the genetic identity of band and scute doublings. Set €.46, rf1 (Table B 2}. The mother shows a reduplicated double scute anomaly in bands 2 and 6 in positions 2 and 3 respectively from the right- hand margin. Fetus I. shows in band 2 an exactly reversed mirror-image reversal of the anomaly in band 2 of the mother and in addition an extensive doubling in the middle of band I. This illustrates three points; (a) reduplication down the primary axis, (b) the genetic relation between scute and band doubling, (c) the half- band type of reversal of symmetry. HEREDITY AND ORGANIC SYMMETRY IN ARMADILLO. TABLE I 3o / C 3 1 9 \ z ^ /- > / TT /£ ->- 'jd i Z. /i /Z ~ i I 3C I ^ 61 > 7 V 23 -> 5 I 3 7- G i i 9 V ri t i- > 7 fur 3o tfb 30 I; id -t 2 !nr 32 ! ^ •. G 23 6 -> 8 C.90? f* 3- J i * S( > ;s 17 T> ; JL 3 f C GG 2C > /2 -j> 6 nr Eg T Q i G 3 3 7 "TO" 8 7 c •} 2 3 3d / -UL 3 6' c > a ^ C 63 £ I 3:€ '3 * -^ 'JIT < 24 3 •d -2 'HE • Q «5~3 k/ i O J-S X HL G : 6'3 6-3, 3L C.729 3d / 30 Al. /3 /3c// 4 G4 C 32 8 56 It should be noted by way of summary that in all cases in which the mother has a band anomaly, one or more offspring of a set have usually a similar band anomaly; but sometimes a scute anomaly is found to be the expression in the offspring of a mater- nal band anomaly. In several cases where there are band anomalies in the offspring a scute anomaly is found in the mother. 26 H. H. NEWMAN. It has been shown clearly in several sets of offspring that there is an extremely close genetic relation between band anomalies and anomalies of single scutes. The two types of anomaly are merely more or less extensive expressions of the same genetic factor. Whenever a mother shows either type of anomaly, one or more offspring show one or the other type. The doubling factor is therefore strikingly dominant in the Mendelian sense. When we find sets of fetuses that show anomalies and the mothers of these show no anomalies, we must conclude that the anomaly is paternal, for a dominant factor in the mother, would appear phenotypically if present genotypically. It is interesting to note that almost the same number of sets have normal as have anomalous mothers. ANOMALOUS OFFSPRING OF NORMAL MOTHERS. There are in the present collection 14 sets of fetuses showing band anomalies, the mothers of which showed neither band nor scute anomalies. In some ways these sets are better for our purposes than are those derived from anomalous mothers, be- cause we can be sure of the uniparental character of the inheri- tance. Since the mothers show no anomalies, those in the off- spring, being unquestionably inherited, must have come from the father. It would be highly interesting to know the conditions in the fathers, but the latter are inaccessible. Since there is no sex difference with regard to the anomalies, we may assume, how- ever, that the same state of affairs would be revealed for the paternal as that made out for the maternal relation. Some of the most interesting cases illustrating the symmetrical distribution of anomalies, intra- and inter-individually, are found among the offspring from normal mothers, and I shall give a complete tabulation of these sets, calling attention in the text only to certain of the more striking conditions, or to situations differing from those dealt with in the sets derived from anomalous mothers. SetK.2, 9 (Fig. 13). This set perhaps better than any other shows the phenomenon of symmetry reversal, or mirror-imaging among the quadruplets. Fetuses I. and III., which face one another across the vesicle, HEREDITY AND ORGANIC SYMMETRY IN ARMADILLO. 2"J and are the secondary bud derivatives of II. and IV. respectively, are exact symmetrical reversals of each other, I. having the anomaly only on the left and III. only on the right. Fetuses II. and IV., the two primary bud individuals, both show the anomaly bilaterally, but are also reversals, in that II. has the more extensive doubling 9-9 on the right and IV. has the more extensive doubling 9-8 on the left. It is interesting to note that the primary individuals II. and IV. are bilateral in their expression of the anomaly, and that the secondary indi- viduals I. and III. are unilateral. Set A. 64, cf (Fig. 14). This set is remarkable for the striking identity of detail exhibited by the anomalies of the different fetuses. Fetus I. has both halves of band I completely double. Fetus II. has both halves partially double and exactly bi- laterally symmetrical, the doubling starting 3 scutes from the margin on each side and extending for 15-15 scutes. Fetuses III. and IV are both exactly like II. on the left side and exactly like I. on the right. There is exactly the same amount of doubling in I. plus II. 28 H. H. NEWMAN. as there is in III. plus IV., but the distribution of the fully doubled and incompletely doubled half bands is different. In the case of the left-hand pair I. and II., I. gets both completely doubled halves and II. gets both incompletely doubled halves; 14 while in the case of III. and IV. both get a completely and an incompletely doubled half. There is no symmetry reversal in this set. Set A. 96, d" (Fig. 15}. Fetus I. has band I double except for 5 single scutes on the right margin. Fetus II. has a similar anomaly bilaterally. Fetuses III. and IV. are very much alike but differ from I. and II. in the extent of the single part of the band, which is much longer than in the other pair. The pairing of fetuses is very clear, but no symmetry reversal is shown. One of four fetuses (II.) is bilateral. Set K. 80, cf (Fig. 16). Fetuses I. and II. are practically identical. In both band I is doubled, except for four single scutes on the left. Fetuses III. and IV. show the incomplete doubling bilaterally HEREDITY AND ORGANIC SYMMETRY IN ARMADILLO. 2 9 16 3O H. H. NEWMAN. with a larger and smaller series of double scutes arranged on opposite sides. Fetus III. has the long series 28-29 six scutes from the right, while fetus IV. has a series of 29-29 scutes 15 scutes from the left and extending past the middle. This is an imperfect reversal of symmetry. There is a better reversal in connection with the short series of double scutes, for in III. the series of 10-12 scutes is situated 5 scutes from the left, and in IV. the series of 9-10 scutes is situated 6 scutes from the right. Set A. 101 (Table 62) has the anomaly confined to one pair of fetuses (I. and II.) and the expression is very different in the two. It is well to note that although the doubling is nearly complete in II. it starts after 6 single scutes at the margin, as is the case on both sides of I., which shows a remarkably exact bilateral symmetry. In the remaining sets, K. 8, K. 12, C. 90, C. 63, C. 69, C. 72, C. 88, C. 32 and C. 41 (Table B2 and 3), band doubling is found in only one fetus of a set. In most sets however scute anomalies, which are usually rather definitely related to the band anomaly, are present in one or more fetuses. Set K. 8 is remarkable for the large amount of reduplication down the axis of scute anomalies, for example fetus III. has double scutes in bands I, 3, 5, 7, and 8, those on bands 5 and 7 being reversals of each other. It is clear that all of the double scutes are in the same region of the band as is the double series in fetus I., and the majority are on the same side of the body. The remaining sets show nothing that has not already been seen in sets previously described. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS. Band anomalies are of the nature of irregularities in the normal regular rows of scutes that make up the typical band, and consist of more or less extensive regional doubling of rows of scutes in a band. This condition is typical for the non-banded parts of the armor but quite rare in the banded regions. It is practically confined to the band or bands nearest the non-banded region. Band anomalies occur in only about three per cent, of individuals and an examination of over two thousand adult individuals taken at random shows no duplicate anomalies. HEREDITY AND ORGANIC SYMMETRY IN ARMADILLO. 3! These anomalies are strongly inherited but are subject to more or less modification. Sometimes a band anomaly unilaterally placed may be inherited unilaterally but with a reversed sym- metry to that of the mother, or it may be bilateral. Sometimes an extensive doubling is inherited both as a similar band doubling, and as a single double scute in the individuals of a single poly- embryonic set of offspring. Contrariwise a double scute in the mother may be inherited as a more or less extensive unilateral or bilateral band doubling. The peculiarity may also be re- duplicated down the primary axis in two or more bands. Frequently in unilateral anomalies the different fetuses of a set show reversed symmetry or mirror-imaging, but it is even more common to find the unilateral anomaly on the same side of most of the individuals, or bilaterally in one or more of them. In a number of cases the anomalies in different fetuses of a set are so strikingly identical as to indicate a rigid predetermina- tion of the details of the character, but in other cases there appears to be only a predetermination of a generalized anomaly that expresses itself to a greater or less extent in the various embryos. In terms of Mendelian inheritance we may say that an anomaly factor is inherited as a dominant character, but its distribution among the fetuses of a set and its location and extent are due to varying ontogenetic or epigenetic factors. The distribution of these inherited anomalies among the various fetuses of the sets furnishes much interesting data, which, together with data on the inheritance of double scutes, to be presented subsequently, furnishes the basis of a discussion of several general questions and especially those involved in the concept of organic symmetry. This subject is reserved for a subsequent paper of the present series. BIBLIOGRAPHY. Newman, H. H. and Patterson, J. T. '09 A Case of Normal Identical Quadruplets in the Nine-banded Armadillo and Its Bearings on the Problems of Identical Twins and of Sex Deter- mination. BIOL. BULLETIN, Vol. 17, No. 3. '10 The Development of the Nine-banded Armadillo from the Primitive Streak Stage to Birth; with Especial Reference to the Question of Specific Poly- embryony. Journal Morphology, Vol. 21, No. 3. 32 H. H. NEWMAN. 'n The Limits of Hereditary Control in Armadillo Quadruplets: A Study of Blastogenic Variation. Journal^Morphology, Vol. 22, No. 4. Newman, H. H. 'i3a The Natural History of the'Nine-banded Armadillo of Texas. American Nat., Vol. 47, Sept., 1913. 'i3b The Modes of Inheritance of Aggregates of Meristic (Integral) Variates in the Polyembryonic Offspring of the Nine-banded Armadillo. Journal Exper. Zool., Vol. 15, No. 2. FACTORS CONCERNED IN THE PRODUCTION OF MITOSIS IN ORGANISMS DISPLAYING CELL CONSTANCY.1 H. J. VAN CLEAVE. The condition of absolute identity in cellular structure found in individuals of many species of Metazoa has led the writer to undertake an analysis of the factors governing mitosis in these forms. Loeb ('12: 4) has called attention to the fact that the first attempt to reduce the phenomena characteristic of life to purely physico-chemical terms is found in the works of Lavoisier and Laplace ('80) which indicated that the heat produced in the body of a warm-blooded animal equalled that given off by a burning candle when the amounts of carbon dioxide produced in the two instances were equal. These results have been so much amplified by later workers that today no one doubts that the general processes having to do with the phenomena of meta- bolism are identical with the so-called purely physical and chemical reactions occurring outside the living body. In fact while metabolism, movement, and irritability are generally granted as probably due to the workings of the same principles that govern the inanimate realm the fourth of the vital properties, that of reproduction, is in great part still unexplained. No problem has offered more secure harbor for a final possibility of the presence of some supernatural force than has this one dealing with the processes of reproduction. The statement of Davenport ('08: i); "The vital processes are chemical processes, taking place in a highly complex, very unstable, constantly changing substance, whose activities we call life," embodies the modern conception of the nature of life phenomena in general, but no attempt has been made to show the actual possibility of explaining all life processes on this basis. An analysis of the possible factors determining or directing 1 Contributions from the Zoological Laboratory of the University of Illinois, No. 43. 34 H. J. VAN CLEAVE. the course of nuclear and cell division is extremely difficult on account of problems involved in eliminating factors until but a single causal agent is operative. However, of the possible factors involved in the production of mitosis and of cleavage two groups are clearly recognizable, namely: (i) Environmental factors, and (2) Internal factors. Any change which occurs within the living organism must find explanation upon the basis of one of these two groups of factors or upon the basis of a com- bination of them. It is the purpose of this paper to show the relationship of these two groups of factors in the production of mitosis among those organisms which are made up of a fixed number of cells. i. ENVIRONMENTAL FACTORS. If environmental factors either caused or directly controlled the phenomena of mitosis the fact of cell constancy could not exist, for no two individuals, and more strongly no two groups of individuals even of the same species, develop under absolutely the same environmental conditions. Consequently if environ- mental factors were the limiting factors in mitosis no two indi- viduals would of necessity contain the same number of cells. It is generally granted that the mitotic process may be accelerated or retarded through the application of purely external stimuli. At least in those organisms made up of a fixed number of somatic cells such an acceleration or retardation of the mitotic process could result in nothing beyond a modification of the normal rate of the process and could in no wise be considered as a direct factor in the determination of the extent of the series of mitotic divisions during the cleavage stages and in subsequent develop- ment. If the role of temperature, for instance, be examined as a possible controlling factor in the mitotic process it becomes apparent that if this alone and directly controlled the number of mitotic divisions through which the developing organism should pass there could be but slight possibility of any two individuals having identical numbers of somatic cells. For under conditions of nature no two individuals are at all times during their develop- ment under absolutely identical temperature relations. A similar variability of conditions for the development of different indi- viduals exists in the case of almost all of the other environmental FACTORS CONCERNED IN THE PRODUCTION OF MITOSIS. 35 factors which might be considered as directly influencing tin- process of mitosis. Furthermore if environmental conditions could directly control and determine the mitotic process, cell division would continue indefinitely through the life of the organism as long as the external conditions remained favorable for it. Again the data brought from the field of cell-constancy show the impossibility of such factors being operative in a controlling manner. In the development of any organism or part of organism the powers of reproduction of most of the somatic cells are restricted to definite periods in the early stages of development, usually preceding the introduction of histological differentiation. Thus in the genus Eorhynclius the writer ('14: 280) has shown that in no instance did he find any adult worm displaying abnormal numbers of somatic nuclei or presenting any evidence whatever of further division of the somatic nuclei after the adult body form had been attained. These observations were upon over two hundred individuals collected during a period of four years at various localities and at various seasons of the year. In conclusion, if the determination of the mitotic process were to find its explanation in terms of environmental factors, or to a continuous production of combinations of environmental factors during the process of development, then in every instance the number of mitotic divisions a fertilized egg would undergo would be a direct resultant of the complex of environmental factors operative during its process of development. At least in those forms with a high degree of cell constancy it seems obvious that purely environmental factors have but one relation- ship to the process of mitosis and that consists in the modifiability of the rate, either as an acceleration or as a retardation. In this respect purely environmental factors have the same general effect upon the process of mitosis as they exert upon purely physical and chemical reactions. 2. INTERNAL FACTORS. Our incomplete knowledge of the finer structure of the cellr and of the nucleus in particular, make it impossible to associate the control of the mitotic process directly with any structure or 36 H. J. VAN CLEAVE. chemical bodies found in the cell. However on the basis of facts pointed out earlier in this paper it seems certain that since there is no possibility of environmental factors acting as the controlling element in mitosis the ultimate cause of the process must be sought within the cell. In this connection invaluable support is found in the field of experimental embryology. Morgan ('95, '01, and '03) and Driesch ('98 and 'oo) have both shown that in the embryos of echinoderms developed from isolated blastomeres of the two cell stage the number of cells present at any point in the development is approximately half of the number present in a normal embryo. Similarly from one of the blastomeres of the eight cell stage the gastrula is composed of only one eighth the number of cells found in the normal gastrula. Loeb ('06: 59) interprets these results as supporting the hypothesis of Sachs ('93 and '95) which regards the factors determining cleavage controlled by the ability of each nucleus to gather around itself and control a definite amount of proto- plasm. Yet what determines the amount of protoplasm present in the developing individual? The cytoplasm is constantly being replaced through the processes of anabolism which experi- ments with enucleated cells have shown to be under the control of the nucleus. Consequently it seems that the view just stated comes not much short of being an argument in a circle. Does the amount of cytoplasm determine the number of nuclei that are to be formed or is the numerical relation of nuclei to cyto- plasm a mutual one brought about not through the influence of •either cytoplasm or of nucleus but through some fundamental factor which determines the number of nuclei and at the same time indirectly the amount of cytoplasm that is to be formed? The writer interprets the data of Morgan and of Driesch in an entirely different manner. If by the first division of the egg there are set apart two units, each of which has the possibility of developing into a given number of cells by the process of mitosis and this tendency is retained, even though the two units become separated, it seems logical to conclude that within the fertilized egg there are resident potencies which through the process of mitosis become divided between the two daughter nuclei of the first and then of each succeeding generation of cells. FACTORS CONCERNED IN THE PRODUCTION OF MITOSIS. 37 As to the nature of this partition with each mitotic division two explanations present themselves. According to the simplest of these mitosis may be the result of the direct chemical activity of certain substances, present in the fertilized egg, which become used up in the mitotic process so that each cell of the two cell stage receives an equal amount of the substance present in the fertilized egg after the amount necessary for the first mitotic division has been eliminated. On this hypothesis with each succeeding mitotic division the materials resident in the de- veloping embryo become partitioned and dissipated in the process of development. Thus if x equals the entire amount of the material for the execution of the mitotic process present in the fertilized egg, and a the amount required for the realization of the first mitotic division then each cell of the first cleavage would receive (x -- a)/2. In this manner each succeeding division would reduce the amount of the material present until the amount apportioned to each cell would be less than the amount required for the execution of the mitotic process, thus bringing about an automatic check upon the course of the series of nuclear divisions. The following objection to such a hypothesis shows its weak point. Upon this basis each blastomere of the early stages would be required to produce the same number of cells, a supposition which the facts of cell lineage do not support. The second explanation of the method of control over the number of divisions of the nucleus seems more natural and does not convey so much of the idea of predestination, in that it requires less emphasis upon the inherent qualities of the fertilized egg. According to this hypothesis mitosis is just as markedly a result of chemical processes going on within the egg as indicated above but the individual cells may in varying degrees retain the power of synthesizing the materials necessary to initiate and carry out the mitotic division of the nucleus. In the application of this explanation the check to the course of the nuclear divisions may come as the result of an accumulation of materials within the cell, probably as metabolic by products, which serve to retard and finally to prohibit the chemical activity incident to nuclear division. Thus at the end of any definite period of physio- logical activity the organism will be composed of a definite 3§ H. J. VAN CLEAVE. number of cells and so upon attaining maturity will have a fixed number of somatic cells which are unable to divide farther on account of the presence of the inhibiting materials. It must be remembered that throughout this discussion principles are being laid down for the determination of the mitotic process in organisms or parts of organisms displaying cell constancy. The objection involving the variability in the number of cells arising from the early blastomeres, cited in connection with the dis- cussion of the influence of environmental factors, finds no grounds here. In fact it lends its support to this second hypothe- sis. If the ultimate check to the mitotic process comes as the result of an accumulation of metabolic by products, then the rate of the nuclear division, which unquestionably may be in- fluenced by environmental factors, would tend but to be directly proportional to the rate of the accumulation of the inhibiting elements, though the actual amount of the inhibitors necessary to terminate the series of mitotic divisions would remain the same. Evidently the accumulation of the inhibitors proceeds unequally in various tissues of the metazoan body. The degree of the differentiation of the cell and, in all probability connected with this, the nature of the cell membranes, determines the rate of the accumulation of the inhibiting factors. The impossibility of explaining the phenomena of cell con- stancy on the basis of mitotic control by environmental factors and the facts from experimental embryology in their bearing upon the control of mitotic division by internal factors, lead to the evident conclusion that during the development of cell constant forms the mitotic process is controlled and determined from within the cell but its rate may be regulated by factors which are considered purely environmental. CONCLUSIONS. 1. At least in those forms displaying a high degree of uni- formity in the number and arrangement of their component cells and nuclei, environmental factors such as ordinarily influence physical and chemical changes play no direct part in the deter- mination of the number of mitotic divisions of the somatic nuclei. 2. The only influence of normal environmental factors in these (••ACTORS CONCERNED IN* THE PRODUCTION OF MITOSIS. 39 forms consists in the modification of the rate of the process of mitosis. 3. The direct control of the mitotic process must be sought in the chemical activity going on within the cells of the developing individual. 4. Experimental evidence indicates that with each cleavage of an egg with determinate cleavage there is retained a definite relationship between the number of any given cleavage and the total number of cleavages that the embryo would undergo even in those cases where the blastomeres become isolated in t he- early stages of development. 5. The foregoing would indicate clearly that within the cells derived from the fertilized egg there are present factors or potencies which exert direct control over the number of mitotic divisions which shall ensue. 6. The fact that not all of the blastomeres of the early cleavages produce the same number of cells indicates that the number of cells produced must be controlled by conditions developing as the process of development progresses rather than by the parti- tion and distribution of some definite materials present in the egg at a time prior to the first cleavage. 7. In tissues which retain the power of continued mitotic division, as for example in the formation of the germ cells and in tissues which have widely varying numbers of cells, the expla- nation of the inconstant nature of the numbers of cells produced may be sought in the acquisition of the power of eliminating from the cell those materials which in the course of the process of metabolism tend to accumulate and serve as inhibitors to the mitotic process. LITERATURE CITED. Davenport, C. B. '08 Experimental Morphology. New York. Driesch, H. '98 Von der Beendigung morphogener Elementarprocesse. Arch. f. Entw.- mech., 6: 198-227. 'oo Die isolirten Blastomeren des Echinidenkeimes. Arch. f. Entw.-mech., 10: 361-410. Loeb, J. '06 The Dynamics of Living Matter. Columbia U. Biol., Ser. VI 1 1. '12 The Mechanistic Conception of Life; Biological Essays. Chicago. 40 H. J. VAN CLEAVE. Morgan, T. H. '95 Studies on the Partial Larvae of Sphserechinus. Arch. f. Entw.-mech., 2: 81-126. '01 The Proportionate Development of Partial Embryos. Arch. f. Entw.- mech., 13: 416-435. '03 The Gastrulation of Partial Embryos of Sphserechinus. Arch. f. Entw.- mech., 16: 117-124. Sachs, J. '93 Physiologische Notizen VI. Ueber einige Beziehungen der specifischen Grosse der Pflanzen zu ihrer Organisation. Flora, 77: 49-81. '95 Physiologische Notizen IX. Weitere Betrachtungen iiber Energiden und Zellen. Flora, 81: 405-434. Van Cleave, H. J. '14 Studies on Cell Constancy in the Genus Eorhynchus. Jour. Morph., 25: 253-299. THE PRODUCTION OF MALES AND FEMALES CON- TROLLED BY FOOD CONDITIONS IN THE ENGLISH HYDATINA SENTA. DAVID D. WHITNEY. It has been found recently in an American parthenogenetic strain of the rotifer, Hydatina senta, from New Jersey that a continuous diet of the colorless protozoan flagellate, Polytoma, causes practically all females to be produced.1 This production of only females can be maintained in this manner through generation after generation for several years. If, however, the diet is suddenly changed to a green protozoan flagellate, Chlamy- domonas, that is in an active state, males can be produced in great numbers. It has been suggested that perhaps this phenomenon of the regulation of the two sexes by food conditions is peculiar to this particular strain of New Jersey Hydatina senta and is not an universal characteristic of the species. Fortunately it has been possible to test this hypothesis on an English strain of Hydatina senta and some very clear and conclusive results have been obtained. I am greatly indebted to A. F. Shull for the stock of English rotifers with the accompanying data. 'The English line was received from Mr. C. F. Rousselet, who collected them as resting eggs in mud at the bottom of a duck pond in England in August, 1912. They were sent to me about November I, 1912, in dry dirt. The first ones hatched a few days later and from them the line sent to you has been reared by parthenogenesis ever since. As they were reared there was a generation about every three days and so you have about the 27Oth generation." The stock of this English line was received from Dr. Shull on January 7, I9I5- The females of this strain produce fewer offspring in the same period of time than the females of the New Jersey strain. This 1 Jour. Exper. Zoo/., Vol. 17, November, 1914. 41 DAVID D. WHITNEY. is probably due to the fact that the former strain had been reproducing parthenogenetically for several years and had become weakened whereas the latter strain was developed from a resting egg in November, 1914, and at the present time is as vigorous as at first. The method of making the media and rearing the two kinds of protozoan food cultures has been given in detail in a former paper and will not be repeated here, excepting a few additional words about the green food. When the Chlamydomonas had been 2-3 weeks in the same media the individuals were large and more or less quiescent. In this condition they were only moderately effective although in one or two instances some that had been two to three months in the same media were very effective. When they had been in new bouillon media for 2-7 days with considerable sunshine at a temperature of about 28° C. they were of various sizes and seemed to be the most effective. However, there seems to be a considerable amount of chance in getting the Chlamydomonas in the optimum condition for every experiment. This probably explains the varying per- centages of the male-producing females obtained in the different experiments. As it seems that the effect of a uniform diet on this rotifer has not been sufficiently emphasized the following Table (I.) con- TABLE I. SHOWING THAT A CONSTANT AND UNIFORM DIET OF THE COLORLESS PROTOZOA, Polytoma, REPRESSES THE PRODUCTION OF MALE-PRODUCING FEMALES AND CAUSES THE PRODUCTION OF FEMALE-PRODUCING FEMALES. Kepi on a Uniform Polytoma Diet. Race. Generation. c?9 9 9 OLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXIX. PLATE 1. - \ -v N \ v N V' Ji -^^VV'-M; -^^m^lL J * ' -/; • / ,/*•/'*' *&** J \r/ r i y /' ' ' *'*%fmi&tJf, '< ''"?/ VV- — - _ "X r- v'^A • x V >/ » k ' ' -i*' ' / JT ^ ^.^ \\jLf' ]\';r^.f^^- i i ' / ' >• >^Tf~' ^-^^ ' '• \i if ( '[,-: V^^^ \ ' • ' ,'. Jv\ ^--i? \ »i.- • "v J^v*^ ^- / \ v\ FIG. i. GEORGE DELWIN ALLEN FIG. 2 128 GEORGE DELWIN ALLEN. PLATE II. FIG. 3. The system of spiral currents. Photograph of pan in which the water has been stirred in the clockwise direction and then drops of carmine have been placed on the bottom at various points about its circumference. The dark lines are streaks of carmine produced by the currents and showing the direction of the currents upon the bottom. FIG. 4. A positive reaction showing individual variation in the precision of orientation. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXIX. PLATE II. FIG. 3. GEORGE DELWIN ALLEN FIG. 4. A CASE OF HERMAPHRODITISM IN SPELERPES BISLINEATUS. CATHARINE LINES CHAPIN, A.M. DEMONSTRATOR IN ZOOLOGY, SMITH COLLEGE, NORTHAMPTON, MASS. Hermaphroditism among Anura seems to be comparatively common. Ecker and Wiedersheim's "Anatomic des Frosches," as revised by Gaupp, devotes several pages to the subject, dividing it into various types, the most common being that in which the glands are essentially male with female elements. Miss King ('10) in her paper on anomalies in the genital organs of toads, says: "Evidently hermaphroditism occurs much less frequently among the Urodela than among the Anura, as only two cases have as yet been reported for this group of Amphibians. La Valette St. George ('95) has given a brief description of a case of hermaphroditism in Triton tceniatus and Knappe ('86) has noted the presence of a Bidder's Organ in a young salamander; neither investigator gives any details regarding the structure of the ovo-testes in these forms." Knappe mentions Triton also in his investigations but seems to have found a Bidder's Organ only in a two-year-old Sala- mandra maculata. Concerning the frequency of occurrence of hermaphroditic salamanders, La Valette St. George says: "Fur die Urodelen liegen, soviel mir bekannt, noch keine Angaben iiber Zwitterbildung vor. "Spengel will solche niemals angetroffen haben, obgleich er zahlreiche Salamander und Tritonen zerlegt und allein von Triton cristatus iiber 100 mannliche Individuen untersucht hat." While collecting data concerning the spermatogenesis and age of attainment of sexual maturity of Spelerpes bislineatiis l I found that a series of sections through the testes of a 46 mm. 1 Although this species has long been called bilineatus, Dr. Green in his origi- nal description of it (Jour. Acad. Nat. Sc. Phila. Vol. I, Pt. II, 1818) gave it the name of Salamandra bislineata. With the change in generic name the masculine ending was adopted and the species is correctly Spelerpes bislineatus. 129 130 CATHARINE LINES CHAPIN. larva, collected in September, 1913, contained several eggs. The larva was nearly of adult proportions and presumably would have undergone metamorphosis the following summer. Fig. i, B, is from a drawing of the pair of gonads and kidneys, made with an Abbe camera before imbedding. It reveals the peculiarities of the glands when compared with Fig. I, A, which is a drawing, at the same magnification, of a normal pair of !o FIG. i. A. Testes and kidneys of normal 48 mm. larva. B. Gonads and kidneys of hermaphroditic 46 mm. larva. C. Ovaries and kidneys of normal 46 mm. larva. These gonads and kidneys are from larvae collected in September. Ventral view, X 75. k = kidney, lo = largest ova, U = left testis, o = ova, ov = ovary, rt = right testis, so = smaller ova, t = testis. testes and kidneys of a 48 mm. larva. The original of Fig. I, B, being merely a hastily made outline drawing and the testes having been very small, I noticed the hermaphroditic condition only on careful study of the sections. A comparison of Fig. i, A and B, shows that, in the abnormal specimen, neither testis is fully developed. The hermaphroditism HERMAPHRODITISM IN SPELERPES BISLIXEATUS. is of two types. Macroscopically, the anterior part of the left gonad, which is much reduced in size, resembles the normal testis in texture, though not in shape, while the posterior region is distinctly like an ovary. The larger ova in this latter region, indicated in outline in Fig. I, B, are six in number. Fig. i, C, is a camera drawing of the ovaries and kidneys of a normal 46 mm. larva, collected in September, showing the arrangement of eggs, for comparison with'their arrangement in the posterior region of the left gonad of the hermaphrodite. In each normal ovary the larger ova lie approximately in two rows, one lateral and one medial. A dozen or fifteen on each side are appreciably larger than the others. These larger ones, as may be deduced from a comparison with the ova in specimens of adult female Spelerpes, collected in September, are of the size and state of development which indicate that they would normally have been deposited a year from the following spring as the first brood of this individual, while the next smaller ones would have been deposited two years from the following spring. The larger ova in the left gonad of the hermaphrodite are about the size of the smaller ova in the normal ovaries, though their irregularity in shape, especially where large numbers of eggs are packed together, makes exact measurements impossible. The ova of both the normal specimen and the hermaphrodite have their long axes parallel to the longitudinal axis of the body. This diameter, as measured from sections, is .15 to .20 mm. while the shorter diameter, at right angles to the long axis, averages about .12 mm. The largest ova found in the herma- phrodite are, in development, a year behind the largest ova in a normal female having the same total length, collected at the same time, and therefore, presumably, of the same age. Beside these ova, there are oogonia in the left gonad of the herma- phrodite, similar to those in the normal ovary which have not yet begun to elaborate yolk (Fig. 2). The relation of ova to follicle cells and to peritoneum in the hermaphrodite is like that in the normal ovary. Although smaller than the right testis, the anterior portion of the left gonad has somewhat the structure of the normal male gland. The elongated mass is composed of too few loulebs to 132 CATHARINE LINES CHAPIN. have the usual arrangement about a collecting duct. The cells, showing no signs of division to form cysts, which is the im- mediate preparation for spermatogenesis, are not as far advanced in their development as the germ cells of a normal male larva several months younger than the hermaphroditic one. The left gonad shows throughout a retarded development. The right gonad, on the other hand, although smaller than the normal testis of an individual of the same size, seems to 0 FIG. 2. Horizontal section through posterior region of left gonad of hermaphro- dite, X 280. / = follicle cell, o = oogonium, ov = ovum, p = peritoneum. be, in general, normal in structure and in its cellular development. However, it shows another sort of hermaphroditism. Two ova are, in this case, found in the otherwise apparently normal testis, each one completely filling one lobule, which would nor- mally contain a large number of male cells (see Fig. 3). These ova are about two thirds the size of those found in the right gonad. All the lobules are divided into cysts, with the excep- HERMAPHRODITISM IN SPELERPES BISLIXKATUS. 133 tion, of course, of the two which contain a large ovum apiece. Most of these cysts are composed of spermatogonia of the last generation, having round or oval nuclei with the chromatin arranged in an irregular network. Nucleoli appear in a few cells of one section. The spermatogonia resemble Kingsbury's Figs, i and 2 ('02). In a few cysts, all the cells are undergoing mitosis. These do not resemble the maturation mitoses described for Desmognathus, and are probably spermatogonial divisions. P FIG. 3. Horizontal section through right gonad of hermaphrodite, < 280. / = follicle cell, / = lobule, subdivided into six cysts of spermatogonia, o = ovum, p = peritoneum, spg = spermatogonium, spin = spermatogonial mitosis. In other cysts the nuclei are in the "contracted" condition, described by Kingsbury (Fig. 18), occasionally occurring in Desmognathus fusca at the beginning of the period of growth of spermatogonia into spermatocytes and more commonly, in (hat species, in the last generation of spermatocytes. Fig. 3 shows an egg filling one lobule and surrounded by follicle cells. In this case, as well as in the left gonad, the relation to follicle cells and to peritoneum is normal. The adjacent lobules show sper- 134 CATHARINE LINES CHAPIN. matogonia, grouped in cysts, surrounded by long follicular cells. The cells of one cyst are undergoing spermatogonial mitosis. A great deal of pigment from the peritoneum appears in this section which was cut near the surface of the testis. The germ cells of the right gonad seem to have reached a normal degree of development. The individual, collected in September, with a total length of 46 mm., would have undergone metamorphosis during the following summer. It is probable that the male Spelerpes attains sexual maturity in the fall after metamorphosis. Then this individual : hould have reached maturity in a little over a year. The spermatogonia are just ready to commence the process of maturation. This process in Desmognathus takes about a year, while Kingsbury's obser- vation that, in Spelerpes, regeneration of the lobule begins before the spermatozoa leave it, indicates that possibly the proc- ess of spermatogenesis requires even more time in Spelerpes. Then it may be supposed that these male germ cells of the right gonad would have become ripe spermatozoa at the normal time. The two ova in this gonad, however, are at least a year behind the normal development. In the left gonad, in which there is a greater abnormality, the development of both kinds of germ cells is retarded. BIBLIOGRAPHY. Ecker, A., & Wiedersheim, R. '96 Anatomic des Frosches, auf grund eigener Untersuchungen durchaus neu bearbeitet von Dr. Ernst Gaupp. Dritte Abtheilung, pp. 347-355- King, Helen Dean. '10 Some Anomalies in the Genital Organs of Bufo lentigenosus and their Probable Significance. Amer. Journ. of Anal., Vol. 10, No. i, January, 1910, pp. I59-I75- Kingsbury, B. F. '02 Spermatogenesis of Desmognathus fusca. Amer. Journ. of Anal., Vol. r. No. 2, February, 1902, pp. 99-135- Knappe, Emil. '86 Das Bidder'sche Organ. Morph. Jahrb., Bd. XL, pp. 489-552. St. George, La Valette. '95 Zwitterbildung beim kleinen Wassermolch (Triton taeniatus). Arch. Mikr. Anat., Bd. XLV., pp. 1-14- NOTE ON THE NATURE AND SOURCE OF "PURPLE X."1 ALVALYN E. WOODWARD. In the summer of 1913, Dr. O. C. Glaser ('14) found (i) that if a suspension of Arbacia sperm be boiled it turns purple, (2) that this purple color disappears if the boiled suspension be allowed to stand over night, and (3) that initiation of development in Arbacia eggs, either by sperm or by egg secretion, can be in- hibited by the addition to the eggs of this boiled suspension so long as it is purple. This purple substance, he provisionally designated as "purple x." I. SOURCE OF "PURPLE X." In 1914, while working with various inhibitors of fertilization in Arbacia, I wished to use "purple x" and found that it did not always appear when Arbacia sperm was boiled. This naturally led to a search for the source of the substance. Sperm was obtained from a large number of males. Each one was rinsed in fresh water, and after the peristome was cut, placed aboral side down in a clean dry watch-glass, until the seminal fluid had been freely shed. A portion of the sperm from each animal was then mixed with filtered sea-water and examined under the microscope and a second, larger portion of each was boiled. It was found that, as a rule, the suspensions which showed fewest foreign cells — blood-cells or fragments of organs — were least likely to form "purple x." Suspensions which appeared, under the microscope, fairly free from foreign cells, could be depended upon to give a colorless filtrate when boiled. What, then, is the impurity which produces "purple x"? By a series of experiments properly checked and repeated, I was able to eliminate successively, the filtered perivisceral fluid (serum), the blood clot which had been washed in sea-water 1 From the Marine Biological Laboratory at Woods Hole and the Zoological Laboratory of the University of Michigan. 135 136 ALVALYN E. WOODWARD. to free it from serum, fresh fluid containing both serum and blood cells, pieces of the alimentary tract, and mesentery. If a piece of fresh mature testis be boiled in sea-water, in sea-water plus sperm, or in distilled water, a purple compound is formed. This color can also be obtained by treating the testis with strong alcohol, which, as observed under the microscope, turns testis cells from greenish brown to purplish. Whether or not this would happen with immature testes could not be determined, since the experiments were carried on during the breeding season. The sperm from one male, when examined under the micro- scope, showed the presence of a few small pieces of testis, and, when boiled, or treated with alcohol, turned purple. The testes were then washed in running sea-water for about three hours, during which they were occasionally pinched with forceps to help free them from sperm. When this was apparently all washed out, a portion of the testis was boiled in sea-water, without giving a purple color. Treating with absolute alcohol, boiling in distilled water, and boiling in sea-water plus fresh sperm also failed to bring out any purple tinge from pieces of the washed testes. The rest were then left standing over night in a finger- bowl of sea-water. After twenty-four hours, it was observed that they tinged the water slightly but distinctly purple. It seejns clear, then, that "purple x" arises from the fresh testis, or from a reaction between testis and sperm. It still remains to be learned whether or not it may be obtained from the ovaries also. II. CHEMICAL NATURE OF "PURPLE X." The best known pigment of sea-urchins is echinochrome, whose chemical reactions are well established. It may be ob- tained from the red blood cells by laking with distilled water, by extracting with alcohol, chloroform or ether. The neutral extracts are a cherry red, the addition of a small amount of NaOH turns them yellowish, and acidulating with HC1 produces a red-" yellow color (MacMunn, '85). Echinochrome was extracted from the washed blood clot by laking with one volume of distilled water. An equal amount of "double sea-water" (sea-water boiled down to one-half its NOTE ON THE NATURE AND SOURCE OF " PURPLE X.' 137 original volume) was then added so that the salt content might equal that of the solution with which it was to be compared. A series of experiments was then run in duplicate, using this echinochrome on the one hand, and "purple x" on the other. The addition of NaOH and HC1 to echinochrome gave the results described by MacMunn. No visible change could be obtained in the "purple x," however, by the addition of either reagent. Thus it was established that, whatever the chemical nature of "purple x" may be, it is not identical with echinochrome. III. PHYSIOLOGICAL EFFECT OF BOILED SPERM. The addition of boiled sperm suspension to eggs, in Arbacia, causes the jelly surrounding the egg to swell, as can be demon- strated by putting them into India ink. It also becomes more sticky. As a result of this, the eggs adhere to each other and to the bottom of the dish. When fresh spermatozoa are added, many get caught in the jelly and form "halos," but some are able to penetrate to the eggs, so that fertilization is not inhibited. However, when "purple x" is present in the boiled sperm, fertilization by fresh sperm and auto-initiation by egg-secretion is inhibited, as shown in the following table. The percentages were all obtained by counting 200 or more eggs, and a vertical column represents eggs from the same female, treated in dif- ferent ways. J Per Cent, of Kggs Divided. A B c D £ F 1 <; Eggs without sperm . . O 84 O O 72 68 0 73 55 0 72.3 76.7 26.6 O P< 82 39 o jlys 43 o o Dermy 56.9 i± Eggs + sperm Eggs + boiled sperm (colorless) + fresh sperm . Eggs + boiled sperm (purple) + fresh sperm . . Eggs + egg secretion + hypertonic after-treat- ment Eggs + egg secretion + boiled sperm (purple) + hypertonic after-treatment . UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN. March 17, 1915. LITERATURE. Glaser, Otto. '14 On Auto-parthenogenesis in Arbacia and Asterias. BIOL. BULL., Vnl. XXVI. MacMunn. '85 On the Chromatology of the Blood of Some Invertebrates. Quart. Jour. Mic. Sci., Vol. 25. ARE FUNCTION AND FUNCTIONAL STIMULUS FAC- TORS IN PRODUCING AND PRESERVING MORPHOLOGICAL STRUCTURE? EDUARD UHLENHUTH, PH.D., ROCKEFELLER INSTITUTE FOR MEDICAL RESEARCH, NEW YORK. Since the days of Lamarck the attempt has often been made of explaining the genesis of the morphological structure of or- ganisms through the theory of adaptation. A special form of this theory is that of "functional adaptation" which was for- mulated under this name byWilhelm Roux about 1880, and was later elaborated by that investigator in an extremely extensive and thorough manner. The most striking organic structures are those which like the bones seem to be constructed on a definitely purposeful plan, offering the largest amount of strength with the smallest amount of material. Other organs, such as the muscles, increase in size as a result of increased function. Roux named this phe- nomenon "functional adaptation," while the structures under- lying this principle he described as "functional structures." He made a number of exceedingly careful anatomical studies of such "functional structures." Endeavoring to explain the genesis of such seemingly purposeful structures from a purely mechanical standpoint, he found that they possessed exactly that construction which was to be expected from a mathematical calculation based on the principle of functional adaptation. In order to make clear the development of functional adapta- tion, Roux fell back upon the most primitive particles of living matter. In his opinion some of these particles have been adapted to respond to functional stimuli, that is, they show a greater tendency to proliferate in the presence of functional stimuli than in their absence. Thus, those elements which were sub- jected to stimuli soon predominated over those which were not thus exposed. If functional structures consisted of such particles, they would FUNCTION AND FUNCTIONAL STIMULUS. 139 show the following characteristics: first, enlargement (functional hypertrophy) with increased stimulus; second, atrophy from inactivity upon cessation of the stimulus; third, regeneration of organs of functional structure to the condition of the original structure would only be possible in the presence of function and functional stimuli (functional regeneration); and fourth, the successful transplantation of functional structures would only be conceivable if after transplantation the organs involved were supplied with an appropriate amount of functional stimulus. The principles, therefore, necessary for an acceptance of the theory of functional adaptation are: functional hypertrophy, atrophy from inactivity, functional regeneration, and functional transplantation. (Various other factors which are similarly instrumental in this connection will be dwelt upon more exten- sively in a subsequent paper.) Should it now appear that these essential principles in so-called functional structures are non- existent, the theory of adaptation would then fail to adequately account for so-called functional structures. Before giving the results of my experiments I wish to say a few words about the general value of Roux's theory of functional adaptation. This theory at a cursory glance has many merits, and at the time when first introduced by Roux it marked a substantial advance, inasmuch as it showed that it was possible to produce highly purposeful structures through the influence of purely mechanical principles. On this basis the theory has become a material factor in connection with the investigations and studies of many workers in the realms of pathology and physiology. Roux's theory, however, has great disadvantages, one of which is its extremely complicated and extensive terminology. It is probably owing to the extreme obscurity of the doctrine that comparatively few investigators, apart from the originator of the theory, have familiarized themselves with it and fully understood its principles. As a result of this practically the only valuable work along this line has been carried on under the direct control of Roux himself, while in contrast with the com- paratively scanty publications emanating from Roux's laboratory an amazingly voluminous mass of literature has been supplied 140 EDUARD UHLENHUTH. by outside writers, all of whom were under the impression that they were contributing something to Roux's theory, but who in reality had hardly grasped more than a few but imperfectly comprehended terms connected with the essential problem. These for the most part misleading publications have caused more error than progress in experimental work. I shall endeavor to prove this point in a subsequent and more extensive article. A second disadvantage of Roux's theory is the fact that the extent of the field in which it is applicable becomes more and more restricted with the increasing number of "experimental" investigations bearing directly upon this problem. Conse- quently, in course of time the phenomena which are not in harmony with this principle of functional adaptation increase in number, although they may be explained, together with other phenomena, from another point of view. However, by far the weightiest objection to the theory is that it threatens to become more and more of a stumbling block to workers who are setting out to investigate the problems of or- ganic and inorganic life from a common viewpoint. Nowa- days theories such as that of function and functional stimulus can hardly be reconciled with a chemico-physical view of the life processes; for the whole underlying principle of the theory of adaptation does not lend itself to methods of measurement. For this reason a detailed revision of the theory of functional adaptation has become necessary and will be published in a later communication. In the present paper I shall report on the results of a few experiments which I began five years ago with the above men- tioned object in view. As they are at the present moment sufficiently advanced to allow of a survey of the whole point at issue and to show that they are qualified to throw some light on the problem of functional adaptation, a preliminary discussion of these experiments may be warranted. The experiments in question were performed on the trans- planted eye of Salamandra maculosa. The eye of a larva was transplanted into the neck of another larva, where after a few days' partial or complete disappearance of the retina resulted, ending finally in complete degeneration. The remarkable fact FUNCTION AND FUNCTIONAL STIMULUS. 14! was that this degeneration was followed later by a complete regen- eration of the transplanted eye, even in the dark. My experiments were conducted with the following considera- tions in view. The retina is one of those structures which ac- cording to Roux's definition should be called a functional struc- ture. But in accepting this definition one need not necessarily assume that these structures are the result of functional stimulus, for such structures may have been predetermined in the embry- onic stage. They develop independently of functional stimulus until they reach the third period — the so-called functional period.1 Functional structures can only exist during this period if they are supplied with a sufficient amount of functional stimulus; otherwise they undergo atrophy through inactivity. From Roux's point of view it would appear probable that the morphogenesis of the eye of Urodela is only partially determined by inheritance, and in accordance with this determination it would reach the same stage of development as that attained by Proteus anguineus. The further development of the eye, com- prising the formation of the rods and cones, that is the true functional parts, would be brought about by the penetration of the rays of light through the skin of the salamander, to its ovaries, and would therefore be the outcome of functional stimulus. Secerov2 asserts that the skin of Salamandra macu- losa permits of the penetration of approximately 1/173 of the quantity of light in which the animal lives. The eyes of Proteus, which inhabits dark caves, must there- fore remain in the primitive Proteus stage, according to the state- ment of Kammerer,3 who believes he has demonstrated that further differentiation can only occur if Proteus be kept in the light. Thus, he ascribes the process of full differentiation 1 See, for example, Roux, "Die Entwicklungsmechanik, ein neuer Zweig der biologischen Wissenschaft," Vortrage und Aufs. iiber Entwickl.-Mech. d. Organ- ismen. 1905, Heft I., p. 94, note n. Also Roux, "Die vier Hauptperioden der Ontogenese, sovvie das doppelte Bestimmtsein der organischen Gestaltungen," Mitteilung der naturforschenden Gesdlschaft, Halle a.d.S., 1911, I., p. i. 2 Secerov, S., "Die Umwelt des Keimplasmas. II. Der Lichtgenus im Sala- mandra-Korper," Arch. f. Enlwcklungsm., 1912, XXXIII. , 682. 3 Kammerer, P., "Experimente iiber Fortpflanzung, Farbe, Augen und Korper- reduktion bei Proteus anguineus Laur. Zgl. Vererbung erzwungener Farbverander- ungen." III. Mitteil., Arch. f. Entwicklungsm., 1912, XXXIII., 349. 142 EDUARD UHLENHUTH. of the eye in the case of several Proteus which were kept in the light, directly to the influence of function, and Roux is apparently of the same opinion. This point will be discussed by the writer in a later paper. My own observations on the transplanted eye of Salamandra soon convinced me that this case lends itself very well for the test of the theory of functional adaptation. First of all I severed the optic nerve, a procedure which ac- cording to general opinion should induce permanent degeneration of the retina as a result of the eye becoming isolated from the brain. In all previous operations of this nature, where, however, the eye remained in its normal position, a reunion of the ampu- tated stumps of the nerve took place, so that it was natural to suppose that the re-connection of the eye with the brain brought about regeneration of the retina. In my own experiments I obviated the possibility of such subsequent reunion of the eye with the brain by transplanting the eye to an abnormal position (in the neck of the salamander). But regeneration took place in spite of this fact. The point of chief interest, however, is the fact that by means of this operative measure, which, as has been demonstrated in 95 per cent, of the cases, excludes reunion of the eye with the central nervous system, the eye is permanently deprived of functional power. It is thus obvious that in these eyes no function was pos- sible and the experiment therefore shows that a whole series of phenomena, hitherto designated as cases of functional adapta- tion, require a different explanation. We will now discuss these phenomena in greater detail. I. In about a week after transplantation of the eye into the neck of the salamander the retina had degenerated to such an extent that in many cases only the peripheral part of the retina, which was not differentiated in layers, had survived.1 But in spite of its permanent isolation from the brain and despite the fact that the eye was permanently deprived of function, the retina from this time on began to show signs of regeneration and the transplanted eye began to receive a progressively im- 1 E. Uhlenhuth, "Die Transplantation des Amphibienauges," Arch. f. Ent- ivcklungsm., 1912, XXXIII. FUNCTION AND FUNCTIONAL STIMULUS. 143 proved supply of blood, so that in a comparativey short time (from 4 to 6 weeks) it had regained a perfectly normal structure. II. This regeneration of the transplanted eye even takes place when the organ is deprived of functional stimulus by light. A series of salamanders operated on in the above-de- scribed manner, were placed in a dark room where neither red nor white light could penetrate to their eyes; but in spite of this fact the transplanted eyes regenerated and developed a normal retina. These experiments show that the "quality" of this process, namely, regeneration as such, is independent of any sort of func- tional influence. We are here dealing with a case of simple regeneration, such as is found in many organs, not with func- tional regeneration, such as we might expect to find in so-called functional structures. Of course this fact does not warrant us in entirely rejecting the theory of functional adaptation, for the possibility must not be ignored that although regeneration occurs in eyes treated in this way as a result of the agency of certain other factors, nevertheless degeneration brought about by atrophy through inactivity might follow later, as a result of the permanent lack of function and functional stimulus, a possi- bility which would be expected to arise according to Roux's theory. III. But secondary degeneration as a result of atrophy from inactivity failed to occur in my experiments, even when the eyes were permanently deprived of function, as occurred in the "light" series. IV. Degeneration similarly failed to occur in the transplanted eyes which were permanently deprived of both function and functional stimulus, namely in the "dark" series. These eyes, although severed from the brain and in permanent darkness, grew and metamorphosed simultaneously with the normal eyes of the hosts.1 Up to the present time I have had at my disposal preparations of eyes of the "dark" series which were preserved 15^ months after transplantation; at that time the hosts were about 21 1 E. Uhlenhuth, "Die synchrone Metamorphose transplantierter Salamander- augen (Zugleich, Die Transplantation des Amphibienauges II. Mitteil.) Arch, f. Entwcklungsm., April, 1913, XXXVI., 211. 144 EDUARD UHLENHUTH. months old, and all the structures, with the exception of the sex organs, were perfectly developed. The retinas of these transplanted eyes were found to be normal in every detail. In addition to the above I have a preparation of an eye of the "light" series, which was preserved 3 1/4 years after trans- plantation, at which time the sex organs were also fully developed. According to Roux the eye of an amphibian should by that time already have entered the functional period, as is believed to have been proved in the case of the eye of the Proteus. Never- theless, the old transplanted eyes were also found to be normal, and the functional parts of the retina, viz., the rods and cones, were present and well developed. The above results, namely, the permanent preservation in a normal condition of transplanted eyes, prove beyond any doubt that the so-called functional structures of the eye do not undergo atrophy through inactivity, even if they are kept under extremely unfavorable conditions and are deprived of all function and func- tional stimulus. This fact alone is sufficient to show that atrophy from inac- tivity, which is one of the fundamental postulates of Roux's theory, is by no means a phenomenon of general occurrence which takes place in all so-called functional structures perman- ently deprived of functional stimulus, as was supposed to be the case. As far as regeneration is concerned, the experiments mentioned above only serve to show that regeneration in itself is indepen- dent of function and functional stimulus. V. I am, moreover, able to demonstrate that the "quantity" of the regenerative process in the eyes, viz., the rapidity of this regeneration are not influenced by functional stimulus, viz., light. A certain number of animals (260 in all) of both the light and the dark series were preserved at certain intervals of time and the transplanted eyes were cut in sections. Eyes preserved at equal intervals of time were then compared with each other. It was found that the transplanted eyes of the same series which had lived on the host for the same period of time may show considerable differences in the rapidity with which they under- go regeneration, even if they are subjected to equal conditions FUNCTION AND FUNCTIONAL STIMULUS. 145 as far as functional stimulus is concerned. These differences must therefore be caused by other non-specific factors. In order to ascertain how far-reaching is the influence of light, it was necessary to determine the average rapidity of regeneration in every group of eyes of the same series and make a curve for each series. Although these curves are not yet completed, the results thus far obtained show no differences between light and dark series at all. Even the quantity of the regeneration is therefore uninfluenced by light. From the above data we must draw the following conclusions: Functional adaptation plays no part either in transplantation or in regeneration of the retina; nor is it a factor which determines either the quality or the quantity of these processes. This, of course, does not mean that regeneration or trans- plantation of the eye cannot be influenced at all by chemical or physical factors. On the contrary, as is shown by the dif- ferences between transplanted eyes of the same series, examined at equal intervals of time after transplantation, the quantity of regenerative processes, viz., the rapidity of regeneration are subject to variation by one or more factors. The point of im- portance is the fact that these factors are not connected with the specific functional stimulus, viz., light. Apparently they are the same factors which also affect the rapidity of the re- generative process of other organs. The factor concerned is probably the length of time since circulation in the transplanted eye was reestablished. Hitherto the regeneration of the retina has been considered as being different from the regeneration of the bones. It was supposed that a bone could only regenerate its architectural structure if in use, otherwise the result would not be a normal bone but a disorderly bony mass. Aug. Bier,1 however, has shown that even the bones do not regenerate as a mere indefinite mass of bone if kept without functional stimulus, but that on the contrary, in the absence of any such stimulus, they resume their original functional struc- ture to the minutest details. In certain experiments a con- siderable part of a human tibia was removed. Although these 1 Aug. Bier, " Beobachtungen iiber Knochenregeneration," Arch. f. klin. Cliir., Dec. 1912, c, 91. 146 EDUARD UHLENHUTH. bones were not exposed to any function, the tibiae after a certain length of time assumed their normal shape and structure, but only if they were supplied with nourishment in a proper way, and if sufficient space was left for them to regenerate the missing part. The same is true of the tendons. H. Triepel1 showed that the tendo Achillis of a cat can regenerate only tendon tissue, irrespective of the presence or absence of functional stimuli. Our own experiments now prove the same principle also in the case of the eye of Salamandra maculosa; this organ regener- ates its functional structures in the absence of functional stimulus, and furthermore it retains its structure permanently, despite the permanent absence of functional stimulus. For a long time it was believed that a bone only regenerated a structureless mass of callus in the absence of function, and according to this theory it would be assumed that an eye if once degenerated would in the absence of light regenerate undifferentiated retina cells, such as we find in the normal Proteus eye. Never- theless, both eye and bone regenerate the normal and fully differentiated structure, even in the absence of functional stimulus. We have seen that in my experiments the velocity of the proc- cess of regeneration was not influenced by function; and even if this had been the case it could not be used as a proof in favor of functional adaptation. There are a number of well known morphogenetic processes, the rapidity of which can be acceler- ated by light, although light bears no relation to the function of the developing organ, that is to say, is not a functional stimu- lus. The most striking experiments made in this connection are those of J. Loeb,2 in which he showed that the regeneration of the hydrants of Eudendrium is impossible in the absence of light. Nevertheless, we cannot call this a case of functional adaptation, because here light is obviously not a functional stimulus. On the other hand, development of the eyes of fish embryos cannot be prevented by the exclusion of the func- 1 Triepel, H., "Selbststandige Neubildung einer Achillessehne," Arch. f. Ent- •wcklungsm., Aug., 1913, XXXVII., 278. 2 Loeb, J., "Uber den Einfluss des Lichtes auf die Organbildung bei Tieren," Pflilgers Arch., April, 1896, LXIII., 273. FUNCTION AND FUNCTIONAL STIMULUS. 147 tional stimulus. But it is incorrect to ascribe this fact to the assumption that heredity may have fixed this character so far that development of an eye now occurs in the absence of light. For Loeb1 has shown that it is very easy to prevent the develop- ment of these eyes by a number of different means, such as lack of oxygen, which is a non-specific, non-functional factor. Moreover, it is not necessary to point out that the influence of light on a photographic plate has never been considered to be a case of functional adaptation, although the sensitiveness of the plate to light is just as much response to a physical factor as is the regeneration of Eudendrium or as might be the variation in the rapidity of eye-regeneration which although not found in our experiments, might possibly have occurred. The theory of functional adaptation complicates instead of simplifying the problem. What we should emphasize is not the fact that the result of the response to light is different in the case of Eudendrium from what it is in the case of the regener- ated eye or of the photographic plate, but the fact that these three phenomena all possess a common basis. It is obvious that all three are governed by the same laws, with which we are familiar from our knowledge of physics and chemistry; but these laws are free from such terms as function, functional stimulus, or any other stimulus, or the principle of adaptation. In order, therefore, to obtain a fertile method for attacking the problems confronting us we must constantly bear in mind the fact that the same laws expressed in the same terms can explain both organic and inorganic phenomena. 1 Loeb, J., "Heredity in Heterogeneous Hybrids," Jour, of Morphol., March, 1912, XXIII., i. Vol. XXIX. September, 1915. No. j BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. II. PHYSICAL CHANGES IN THE EGG OF ARBACIA. L. V. HEILBRUNN. CONTENTS. I. Physical Organization of the Egg 149 II. Cortical Changes 158 A. Membrane Elevation and Membrane Swelling 158 B. Permeability Changes in the Vitelline Membrane 160 C. Theories of Membrane Elevation 163 D. Cortical Action of Heat and of Various Chemicals 169 E. Inhibitors to Membrane Swelling 174 F. Cortical Changes at Fertilization 178 G. The Significance of Cortical Change 183 III. Internal Changes. The Problem of Segmentation 184 A. Theories of Segmentation 184 B. The Action of Hypertonic Sea-water in the presence of KCN 185 C. An Analysis of the Methods of Producing Segmentation in the Unfertilized Arbacia Egg 188 IV. Summary 200 V. References 201 I. PHYSICAL ORGANIZATION OF THE EGG. In spite of the many researches on the sea-urchin egg very little is known of its physical make-up. Year after year the egg is used in attempts to analyze fundamental problems and various theories have been based upon experiments with it. And yet, but little is known definitely about the type of physical organization which it possesses. Is the egg essentially fluid or is it a more or less rigid jelly, is the unfertilized egg surrounded by a microscopically visible membrane or by a hypothetical film beyond the limits of vision, what indeed is the nature of the membrane which controls osmotic interchange? These 149 150 L. V. HEILBRUNN. are some of the questions which must be considered if our theories are to be more than mere generalizations. The first point to be decided upon is the physical nature of the egg contents. Embryologists in the past have often ob- served flowings in egg cytoplasm and such observations indicate a fluid composition. This would accord with Rhumbler's demonstration of fluidity in other types of protoplasm. We might accept these results without further comment if it were not for the fact that recently there has been a tendency to regard protoplasm as a gel. Kite ('13) has in fact concluded that the protoplasm of the starfish egg is of this nature. The protoplasm of the normal unfertilized sea-urchin egg is undoubtedly in a typically fluid condition. If pressure is applied to eggs under- neath a coverslip, the egg contents flow out, indeed, if the pres- sure is vigorous enough, the protoplasm is shot out in a long stream as from a pipette. Facts such as these are probably known to many embryologists, similar observations have been especially described by Reinke ('95) and Albrecht ('98). If the egg is essentially a fluid mass, what is there to prevent it from diffusing through the sea-water? Two possibilities exist, either (i) The substance of the egg is as a whole insoluble in sea-water or (2) it is surrounded by a membrane insoluble in sea-water.1 The first possibility is excluded by the fact that we know protoplasm to be in aqueous solution. We must there- fore conclude that the egg is surrounded by a membrane insoluble in sea-water. Careful observation reveals the existence of a membrane around the unfertilized egg; just outside of the darker substance of the egg cytoplasm, a dim outer line can usually be made out. The faintness with which the outer margin of this vitelline membrane appears is apparently due to the fact that its refractive index is very close to that of sea-water. That the appearance of a membrane is not the result of a diffraction illusion is shown by the fact that the membrane may be isolated by pressing out the egg contents, as Herbst first found. At fertilization, as will be pointed out more fully later, it is this 1 The oft-made assumption of a surface film like that found at the surface of peptone solutions, etc., is really a special case of the first alternative. For such a film could only exist at a surface of discontinuity, and this could only occur at the junction of 2 immiscible fluids. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 15! membrane which becomes lifted from the surface of the egg as the fertilization membrane. A study of the chemical behavior of the membrane gave results of interest. Dilute acids cause it to swell. When one part of normal HC1 is diluted with 9 parts of sea-water the resultant solution causes a marked swelling. The membrane becomes sticky and agglutination follows.1 Dilute solutions of nitric, butyric, and valerianic acids give similar results. As far as can be ascertained however, this acid swelling of the mem- brane does not result in complete solution. On the other hand dilute alkaline solutions, although they cause little if any swelling of the membrane, are quite effective in dissolving it away. In order to study the effect of alkali on the membrane, it is best to shake the jelly off the eggs first, as this often becomes saturated with the Mg (OH)2 precipitated by the alkali, and obscures the result. Eggs in 50 c.c. of sea- water plus 2.5 c.c. n/io NaOH soon become sticky; they cling to the bottom of the dish and to each other. Soon the exterior surface of the egg becomes rough, it is evidently no longer surrounded by a membrane (and it is only prevented from diffusing through the sea-water by coagulation). Many salts exert a swelling effect on the membrane, and in some cases this is accompanied by a complete solution of the membrane. When eggs are dropped into 0.55 N Nal, the swollen membranes are very ap- parent after 10 or 15 minutes have elapsed. In 40 minutes almost all of the eggs are no longer surrounded by a membrane, the periphery of these eggs instead of being smooth, now pre- sents a roughened appearance. It is quite evident that the protoplasm is naked and that the membrane has been completely dissolved away by the sodium iodide. This behavior of the membrane towards acids, alkalis, and salts, indicates its protein nature. That it is not a lipoid is evident from the fact that it is insoluble in any of the ordinary lipoid solvents. It might however contain an admixture of lipoids. This is rendered improbable by the following line of evidence. As was pointed out above, the almost invisible character of the membrane indicates that its refractive 1 Loeb ('08) described agglutination in HC1. 152 L. V. HEILBRUNX. index is very close to that of sea-water. Now it is well known that many proteins have a refractive index close to that of sea- water; on the other hand, lipoids have a considerably higher refractive index. As the refractive index is an additive function of the constituents of a mixture, the presence of lipoids in any abundance would make it impossible for the refractive index of the membrane even to approach that of sea-water. Hence no great admixture of lipoids can be present. More direct evidence of the absence of any appreciable quantity of lipoids is also avail- able. The membrane was tested with a Scharlach R solution such as recommended by Herxheimer. In order to render them more visible, the membranes were made to swell by placing the eggs in 15 c.c. sea-water plus 10 c.c. 2.57VNaCl. To a drop of Scharlach R solution on a slide was added a drop of egg suspen- sion. The vitelline membrane remained hyaline and unstained, whereas the egg cytoplasm itself was colored red. The Scharlach R solution used in this test was a saturated solution of the dye in equal parts of acetone and 70 per cent, alcohol. If the egg contents be made to flow out from the membrane or if the egg be cut or shaken into fragments, a new membrane immediately forms around the momentarily naked protoplasm.1 Such a membrane has the same chemical properties as the vitel- line membrane. The acids and salts which produce swelling in the latter, cause it to swell also. The immediate production of a protein membrane about these egg fragments must be re- garded as similar to the formation of precipitation membranes like those studied by Traube and Quincke. Evidently, some protein contained in the egg is precipitated on contact with the outer sea-water. It is probable that this protein is, in the in- terior of the egg, prevented from coagulation by the presence of a protecting colloid. At the outer surface of the egg, the mem- brane-protein is coagulated by direct contact with sea- water. Support for this view is found in the fact that the presence of some colloids (e. g., egg albumen) in the sea-water, causes the membrane to take up water and swell. Since the vitelline mem- 1 O. and R. Hertwig in their " Untersuchungen zur Morphologic und Physiologic der Zelle," Heft 5, 1887, first observed the elevation of membranes on egg frag- ments. (This observation was also repeated by Ziegler, Arch. f. Ent. Mech., VI., 249 (1898), by Moore, Univ. of California Publications in Physiology, IV., 89 (1912). STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 153 brane behaves toward reagents in the same manner as does the membrane on egg fragments, we are justified in regarding it too as a precipitation membrane. The most noteworthy feature of the precipitation membranes of Traube and Quincke is their semipermeable (or partially semi-permeable)1 character. \Ye should therefore expect the vitelline membrane to exhibit semi- permeable properties, and in this way to govern osmotic inter- change. When the egg is caused to shrink in a hypertonic solution, the vitelline membrane shrinks with it. This has been observed in a great variety of hypertonic solutions. For such a shrinkage, there are 3 possible types of explanation. The inward tension may be the result of a force arising from (i) the substances within the membrane, (2) the membrane itself, or (3) something immediately outside of the membrane. Physico-chemically the possibilities, as I see them, are (i) The fluid interior of the egg is coagulated by the various hyper- tonic solutions, shrinks as a result and pulls the membrane with it. (2) The vitelline membrane as a result of its semipermea- bility2 is responsible for the shrinkage. (3) There is an invisible semipermeable membrane outside of the vitelline membrane. As there is nothing to warrant the assumption of any semiper- meable membrane outside of the vitelline membrane, it seems scarcely necessary to discuss the third possibility. The first possibility is essentially the position maintained by M. Fischer ('io). He regards the passage of water into and out of cells as due primarily to the attraction of the interior colloids for water. According to this view endosmosis, such as occurs in hypotonic solutions, would be the result of the taking up of water by col- loids within the cell. That the process of water absorption is by no means dependent on the affinity of the egg colloids for water is conclusively proven by the fact that influx of water 1 The existence of an absolutely semipermeable precipitation membrane (i. e., one which prevents the passage of all substances in solution, but permits the passage of solvent) is extremely doubtful; cf. for example Quincke '02. The fact that a substance may penetrate a membrane and yet exert considerable osmotic pressure against it, has often been neglected by biologists. To assume that because a substance passes through a membrane it can exert no osmotic pressure against it, is just as foolish as to assume that the air in an air-bubble exerts no pressure on the film of water surrounding it. - Using the term in its broadest sense. 154 L- v- HEILBRUNN. leads not to transformation of gel to sol, as we would have to sup- pose, but on the contrary such an influx transforms the egg pro- toplasm from the sol to the gel condition. The experimental evidence in support of this fact is given on p. 195. Water absorp- tion by the egg is indeed correlated with a loss of water on the part of the egg proteins. The passage of water into and out of the cell can not therefore be due to changes in the water content or aggregation state of the egg proteins, and the first possibility is unable to account for the facts. Only the second possibility remains, i. e., that the vitelline membrane acts as a semiper- meable membrane and controls osmotic intercourse. On the basis of this view many facts are understandable, which can be explained in no other way. This will, I think, be demonstrated in the course of the argument. The plasma-membrane of a cell is defined as the membrane which governs its osmotic intercourse. According to this defi- nition, the vitelline membrane is the plasma-membrane of the Arbacia egg cell. Hitherto no one has either described a plasma membrane, or studied directly the properties of one. Although he often uses the concept of such a membrane, Lepeschkin ('n) admits that the actual structure is "zurzeit unbekannt." Loewe, ('13) in referring to the plasma-membrane, says: '"Allein weit davon entfernt. dasz auch nur ihre Existenz irgendwie sicher- gestellt ware, sind auch iiber die Beschaffenheit dieses hy- pothetischen Gebildes die Meinungen so zahlreich wie die Moglichkeiten." Fischer ('12) is indeed of the opinion that "the entire conception of an osmotic membrane about cells is an impossibility." The plasma-membrane of the Arbacia egg is a protein gel. As such, it possesses a certain degree of rigidity. Suppose a hypothetical system completely surrounded by an extremely rigid semipermeable membrane.1 If such a system were placed in a concentrated solution no exosmosis could take place, for if the membrane were perfectly rigid, there could be no removal of solvent from the system without the production of a vacuum. But the membrane would be subjected to a considerable pressure, which would tend to make it rearrange its particles in such a 1 Possibly this is the case in the Fundulus egg. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 155 fashion that the volume enclosed within it might be lessened. Whereas an extremely rigid membrane would resist such forces, one with only a certain degree of rigidity would yield (in the case of sufficient pressure), and exosmosis would be possible. Thus osmosis in an enclosed system depends, to some extent at least, on the rigidity of the confining membrane. These conclusions apply in some measure to the sea-urchin egg, for the vitelline membrane possesses a slight degree of rigidity. Most salts in hypertonic solution cause the membrane to absorb water and swell, the gel becomes less stiff, and the particles of the mem- brane can more readily rearrange themselves. Thus shrinkage of the egg is favored. We should in fact expect that hyper- tonic solutions of salts which cause membrane swelling would be more effective in causing shrinkage than those which do not. Direct evidence of this fact is difficult to obtain without in- troducing complications. One might compare the shrinkage of the egg in two solutions of equal osmotic strength, one of which causes membrane swelling and the other of which does not. But we have no means of knowing when two solutions are of equal osmotic power. Vant Hoff's law does not apply with sufficient accuracy to warrant its use,1 and even if two solutions could be obtained which were isosmotic towards a given membrane, they would not necessarily be isosmotic towards the plasma-membrane of the Arbacia egg, which is very probably only partially semipermeable. Fortunately theie is a way out of the difficulty. I found that when 2.5 N NaCl was added to sea-water in the proportion of 8 parts by volume of the former, to 50 of the latter, the resulting hypertonic solution would usually cause membrane swelling when freshly prepared, but would in large measure lose this power after it had stood for some time. Thus one can obtain two solutions of identical strength, one of which produces a softening effect on the mem- brane, the other lacking this effect. A number of experiments showed that in every case the eggs shrank more in the solution which caused membrane swelling than in the solution which left the membrane with its original rigidity. Two sample experiments are recorded here. To save time the term "NaCl 1 Cf. Findlay, "Osmotic Pressure," London, 1913 (Chapt. IV.). 156 L. V. HEILBRUNN. hypertonic sea- water" is used to designate 50 parts (by volume) of sea-water plus 8 parts of 2.5 N NaCl. August 21. Two small stender dishes, A and B, were used. A contained 29 c.c. of NaCl hypertonic sea-water made up on August 1 8, B contained 29 c.c. of NaCl hypertonic sea-water freshly prepared. At 10:04 A.M., 5 drops of egg suspension were added to A, and a similar amount to B. The following measurements of egg diameters were made at the times indicated. In making these measurements a Spencer movable scale micrometer was used. It was not found possible to obtain any very great accuracy in the use of this instrument. If the eggs are not subjected to pressure of the coverslip, they tend to move their position slightly. After various attempts I reached the conclusion that the use of the movable scale was inadvisable, especially as the usual difficulties of focusing made very accurate measurements out of the question. Accordingly the measurements were made with the scale stationary. No great claim for accuracy is therefore made, but the error is not over one micron. Fortunately the difference between the diameters of the two sets of eggs is markedly greater than the experimental error of the method. The measurements were made at a magnification of about 650 diameters. DIAMETERS OF EGGS IN A. 70 /j. 70.5 M at 10.25 A.M. 69 71 at 10.28 A.M. 69 70 at 10.29 A.M. 67.5 69 at 10.30 A.M. 69 69 at 10.51 A.M. 68 71 at 10.53 A.M. 68 71 at 10.55 A.M. 69 69 at 10.57 A.M. 68 69 at 10.58 A.M. Average 69.2^. DIAMETERS OF EGGS IN B. 66 fj. 66 M at 10.32 A.M. 67 68 at about 10.35 A.M. 66 67 at about 10.35 A.M. 66 66 at about 10.35 A.M. 66 66 at ii. 15 A.M. 70 70.5 at 11.17 A.M. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 157 The above egg showed a completely unswollen membrane, and so should be ex- cluded from the experiment. 63.5 65 at about 11.20 A.M. 67 67.5 at ii. 24 A.M. Average 66.2 /j. (egg with unswollen membrane excluded). The eggs used in the above experiment were all from a single female. The normal untreated eggs of this female were practi- cally spherical and measured 74. 5M, 75M, 75/*i 74M, 75M, 75M, 75/". 75Mi 75-5M> 75M- They thus possessed an average diameter of 75/x and were practically all of the same size. The experiment shows that the eggs in B with swollen membranes shrank more than did the eggs in A with unswollen membranes.1 The average decrease in diameter of the former was about 9^1, of the latter about 6^. The difference was also constant, for the largest egg with swollen membrane was smaller than the smallest egg with unswollen membrane. August 23. Two small stender dishes were used. A con- tained 29 c.c. of "NaCl hypertonic sea-water," which had been made up on August 18 (at n 130 'P.M.). B contained 25 c.c. of sea-water. At 9.55 A.M., 4 drops of an egg suspension were dropped into A and the same amount in B, and then 4 c.c. 2.5 TV NaCl were added to B, so that this solution became "NaCl hypertonic sea-water." The diameter of the eggs was determined as in the previous experiment. At 10:25 it was noticed that some of the eggs in A were beginning to acquire swollen membranes, so that only a few measurements were made after this time, and only those eggs with unswollen mem- branes were selected. DIAMETER OF EGGS IN A. 65 fj. X 65 /j. at 10.12 A.M. 65 66 at 10.13 A.M. 67.5 70 at 10.14 A.M. 65 70 at 10.15 A.M. 65 68 at 10. 16 A.M. 63.5 X 70.5 at 10.17 A.M. 65 66 at 10.18 A.M. 1 This result was obtained in spite of the fact that membrane swelling always tends to produce an increase in egg volume. Whenever membrane swelling occurs in isotonic solutions, the egg rapidly increases its volume, it cytolyzes. The cause of this cytolysis resulting from membrane swelling will be considered later. X 68 at 10.19 A.M X 65 at 10.20 A.M X 66 at 10.26 A.M X 69 at 10.30 A.M X 68 at 10.31 A.M X 67 at 10.32 A.M Average 66.5 , u.. 158 L. V. HEILBRUNN. DIAMETER OF EGGS IN A. 65 65 66 66 66 66 DIAMETER OF EGGS IN B. 63 n X 63.5 M at 10.04 A.M. 63 63 at 10.05 A.M. 64 65 at 10. 06 A.M. 63 64 at 10. 06 A.M. 64 65 at 10.07 A.M. 62 65 at 10.08 A.M. 63 63.5 at 10.08 A.M. 62 62 at 10.09 A.M. 63 63.5 at 10.10 A.M. 63.5 64 at 10. ii A.M. 64 65 at 10.34 A.M. 63.5 63.5 at 10.35 A.M. 63.5 66 at 10.36 A.M. 63.5 65 at 10.37 A.M. 63 63.5 at 10.38 A.M. 62 66 at 10.39 A.M. 64 64 at 10.40 A.M. 63.5 65 at 10.41 A.M. 63.5 64 at 10.41 A.M. 63.5 65 at 10.42 A.M. Average 63.7 /j.. In this experiment also only eggs from a single female were used. The untreated eggs measured 72/4 x 73^1, 72^1 x 73, 7iju x 73M». 7I-5M x 74/z, 7i.5/zx73/z. Considering the eggs as spher- ical, their average diameter was 72.4^. Thus on the average, the diameter of the eggs in A decreased 5-9/i, whereas the diame- ter of the eggs in B decreased 8-7/i. Thus exosmosis was much more pronounced in the solution which caused membrane swelling than in a solution of equivalent concentration in which this effect was lacking. II. CORTICAL CHANGES. A. Membrane Elevation and Membrane Swelling. When the sea-urchin sperm comes in contact with the egg, almost immediately the vitelline membrane is lifted away from STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 159 the egg surface. This is the well-known process of membrane elevation, or, as it is usually spoken of in this country, membrane formation. During elevation, the membrane under normal conditions does not undergo any evident increase in thickness. The inner border of the vitelline membrane is often not very plainly visible. In order therefore to estimate the thickness of the membrane after elevation, it is convenient to compress the eggs gently. The egg then becomes pushed out against the vitelline membrane, in some directions at least, and the distance between its outer border (i. e., the hyaline layer, see below) and the outer boundary of the vitelline membrane is a measure of the greatest possible thickness of the membrane. Under such conditions, high power examination showed that the thickness of the elevated membrane is approximately the same as that of the unelevated membrane. Quantitative measurements were not found to be practicable. Moreover, after elevation the membrane still retains the same chemical properties that distinguished it before fertiliza- tion. In dilute HC1 it swells rapidly, and soon becomes sticky. Nal also induces the elevated membrane to swell, much as it did before fertilization. After the vitelline membrane has been lifted from the egg surface, a new membrane appears around the cytoplasm. This structure has received an unusually large number of names; of these I shall use the term "hyaline layer." It seems reason- able to conclude that its formation depends on the same pre- cipitation reaction which produces the membrane about egg fragments, and which is probably also responsible for the vitel- line membrane. This conclusion is supported by the fact that it shows semipermeable properties. The process of membrane swelling has often been confused with that of membrane elevation. This is perhaps due to the ambiguity of the term membrane formation. It has been the custom to apply the term whenever the observer notices some- thing at the egg surface which he did not see at the beginning of the experiment. But membrane swelling and membrane elevation are two very different processes and are usually easily distinguishable under the microscope. The elevated 160 L. V. HEILBRUNN. membrane appears as a thin membrane at some distance from the egg surface, which is now bounded by the above-mentioned hyaline layer. On the other hand, the swollen membrane appears as a homogeneous layer surroundng the egg, a layer in which neither the inner boundary of the vitelline membrane, nor the outer boundary of the hyaline layer, make their appearance. In addition to the purely morphological differences, there are other distinguishing features. Among these may be mentioned the fact that swollen membranes are always sticky, and as a result, eggs with such membranes tend to agglutinate. Normal elevated membranes are never sticky. Another criterion depends on the fact that elevated membranes collapse when placed in a solution of egg albumen (or other colloid).1 Swollen membranes are of course unable to collapse. B. Permeability Changes in the Vitelline Membrane. The elevated membrane is known to be readily permeable to electrolytes.2 Hence, since it offers considerable resistance to their passage before elevation, it must undergo a change in permeability at some stage in the process. An attempt was made to determine if this increase in permeability took place before or after membrane elevation. In the first case, it might be considered as causally related to the process, and R. Lillie has in fact suggested that the cause of membrane elevation is an increased permeability of the "plasma-membrane." Ex- periments, however, have shown that the increase in permea- bility follows rather than precedes membrane elevation. Im- mediately after elevation, the membrane is still more or less impermeable to electrolytes. The following experiments show this to be the case: August 21, 1913. Eggs from a single female were washed twice and then gathered into about 10 c.c. of sea- water at the bottom of a small beaker. Four or five drops of diluted sperm were then added, and the beaker shaken. At intervals of i, 2, 3, 4, 5 minutes after insemination, the eggs were re- 1 It is only some few minutes after elevation that this collapse can be produced by a colloid. Cf. p. 162. 2 Cf. Loeb, "Artificial Parthenogenesis and Fertilization," p. 208. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. l6l moved with a pipette and dropped into Syracuse dishes filled with 2 M MgCl2. Just two minutes after insemination, a necessarily hasty examination of the eggs in the beaker showed that all the eggs had well-elevated membranes at this time. On the other hand, eggs removed from the beaker at the same time and placed in 2 M MgCU, upon later examination, showed no signs of ajt elevated membrane, and the membrane had evidently been pushed back against the egg. In the following table, the fractions in the second column indicate the proportion of eggs which showed the membrane elevated in the various Syracuse dishes. In each case, the numerator denotes the number of eggs with membranes elevated, the denominator the total number of eggs counted. Minutes after Insemination Membranes Elevated Before Transfer to 2 M MgCk. (Free from Egg). 1 0/50 2 0/50 3 1/50 4 30/50 5 49/50 In the case of the eggs transferred to the MgClo solution three minutes after insemination, some of the membranes were not completely collapsed, but one, two, or even several small globular expansions could be detected at the egg surface. This experiment was repeated on August 22, 1913, with almost identical results. In this case the eggs were transferred to 2 M MgClo at intervals of 2, 3, 4 minutes after insemination. At i^ minutes after insemination hasty observation showed all the eggs to have well-elevated membranes. Thus the eggs were placed in the magnesium chloride solution after membrane elevation had occurred. Nevertheless, as the following table shows, the eggs removed to MgCl2 two and three minutes after insemination, showed no membranes free from the egg. After 2 minutes, 0/50 with membranes free from egg. 3 i/50 4 13/50 These experiments were also confirmed in the summer of 1914. I have interpreted the results as indicating that after elevation the membrane still remains impermeable to MgClo l62 L. V. HEILBRUNN. (or as would no doubt be a more exact expression of fact, the membrane still offers sufficient resistance to the passage of MgClo so that this [salt exerts osmotic pressure against it). Indeed after being collapsed by the MgCl2, the previously elevated membrane behaves as a semipermeable membrane and shrinks with the egg. That the membrane is still "semipermeable" shortly after elevation is also shown by another series of experiments. It was found that the well-known collapse of the elevated membrane in solutions of egg albumen, did not take place immediately after elevation. In an experiment of July i, 1914, eggs were inseminated in a Syracuse dish at 9:43 A.M. Two drops of egg suspension were then placed 'in various Syracuse dishes containing 10 c.c. of i per cent, egg albumen1 in sea-water, at intervals of 1,3, 6, 12^ minutes after insemination. In the case of the eggs placed in the albumen solution i and 3 minutes after insemination, the membrane was well elevated and had suffered no collapse. On the other hand, when the eggs which had been placed in the albumen solution 6 and \2\ minutes after insemination were examined, their membranes were found to have been bent back and collapsed. This experiment was repeated a number of times, similar results being obtained in each case. These surprising results find an easy explanation on the ground that the membrane is still partially permeable shortly after elevation. Because of this semipermeability it is subjected to the pressure of the electrolytes contained within it. Only when the membrane loses its semipermeable properties does this pressure cease and only then can the albumen cause its collapse. The albumen seems to exert a protective effect on the membrane and to prevent increase of permeability. Thus, when eggs were placed in albumen i minute after insemination^ their membranes were found to retain their semipermeability and they collapsed upon being placed into 2 M MgClo 14 minutes after insemination. From these experiments we can, I think, conclude that in- creased permeability follows upon, rather than precedes, mem- 1 Kahlbaum's crystallized egg albumen was used. The solution was filtered. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 163 brane elevation. At a later point, in discussing what I believe to be the true explanation of membrane elevation, I shall en- deavor to point out a reason for this sequence. C. Theories of Membrane Elevation. The problem of membrane elevation is quite distinct from that of segmentation, which is the central theme of the study of artificial parthenogenesis. No method of producing mem- brane elevation ordinarily results in more than one or two per cent, of segmentations, and many methods usually result in no segmentations whatsoever (e. g., chloroform, urethane). Moreover, some of the best methods for producing segmentation do not involve membrane elevation: thus hypertonic sea-water never results in an elevation of the membrane. Of late Loeb has been of the opinion that membrane "forma- tion," involves the swelling of some substance at or near the egg surface. To quote from his book1 "a colloidal substance which lies below the surface layer of the unfertilized egg or is secreted from the egg, suddenly swells by absorption of sea-water. In the typical case of membrane formation this swelling results finally in a complete liquefaction of the colloid. In other cases the swelling is less complete and the formation of a gelatinous film results." Loeb thus regards the process of membrane formation as due to the swelling of a colloid, concerning the position of which he is not quite clear. On page 213 of the same book he says it is in the cortical layer. Finally, if the egg is left too long a time in a solution which causes membrane formaton, not only a colloid at the periphery, but colloids throughout the egg swell, and cytolysis results. In the discussion of Loeb's theory, I shall assume that he uses the word "swell" in its colloid-chemical sense, i. e., to de- note the absorption of a liquid (in this case water) by a gel. Of course any less specific use of the term would rob the theory of all theoretical significance. Briefly, the objections to the swelling theory, as upheld by Loeb, are: I. All the reagents which cause membrane elevation can scarcely induce the swelling of any one colloid. Loeb claims 1 Loc. cit., p. 210. 164 L. V. HEILBRUNN. that some of the "membrane-forming substances" cause the egg membrane of the Mollusk Lottia to swell, but he admits that this is not true of all of them. Some seem to cause swelling and final liquefaction of the chorion or jelly of the sea-urchin egg, but here again all are not effective, and I can suggest chloroform as an exception. In fact it must be a strange colloid which can be made to absorb water by such reagents as distilled water, alcohol, chloroform, toluol, picric acid. 2. It is difficult to conceive of the location of the swelling colloid. The egg has been shown to consist essentially of a more or less rigid membrane surrounding a mass of fluid contents. Evidently the latter can not swell, as only gels possess this prop- erty. It is also demonstrable that the outer vitelline membrane itself does not swell in the case of true membrane elevation, for it can scarcely be doubted that the vitelline membrane undergoes an increase rather than a decrease of rigidity after elevation. And swelling is always correlated with a decrease in rigidity on the part of the gel. It is difficult to understand how Loeb seeks to explain by swelling what he regards as the formation of a more or less rigid membrane. 3. Cytolysis results not in colloidal swelling and liquefaction, but in coagulation. Loeb considers membrane elevation and cytolysis as due to the same processes. He says:1 "Substances like benzol, saponin, etc., can cause both membrane formation and cytolysis. The first of the two is produced when they have time to affect only the surface of the egg; cytolysis is produced when their effect extends to the deeper layers of the egg . . . the greater the fraction of the egg which conies under the effect of the membrane-forming reagents, the greater the amount of colloid that must be liquefied." Loeb thus states explicitly that the membrane-forming reagents "liquefy" the colloids of the eggs, and that this effect in the case of cytolysis extends into the interior. Now it is a fact that the membrane- forming reagents, far from producing liquefaction, have an ex- actly opposite effect upon the colloids of the Arbacia egg. Instead of transforming a solid mass to a more fluid state, what they really do is exert a solidifying or rather a coagulating effect 1 Loc. cit., p. 213. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 165 upon the egg colloids. The actual evidence in support of this statement will be given at a later point (p. 196). In the first paper of this series, after showing that all sub- stances which produce membrane elevation cause a lowering of surface tension, I proposed a theory of membrane-elevation based on a simple consideration of the forces in equilibrium at the vitelline membrane. At the time I was not yet aware of the semipermeable nature of this membrane, and hence in discussing the various forces acting upon it, I did not consider osmotic forces as being involved. The theory therefore re- quires slight modification in the light of this new fact. Let FIG. i. us suppose in Fig. I that the shaded area is the vitelline mem- brane. The arrows represent the forces acting upon it. Arrow A directed outward indicates the outward force due to the osmotic pressure of the dissolved substances within the membrane. Were gels present in the egg we should also have to add the swelling pressure of these. Directed inward are two arrows, arrow B is the force due to the osmotic pressure of the salts outside the membrane, arrow C is the radial component of the surface tension of the membrane. As the membrane is slightly thicker than twice the range of molecular action, it is really composed of two surfaces of surface tension, i. e., an outer surface of contact with the sea-water and an inner surface of 166 L. V. HEILBRUNN. contact with the egg contents. Both surfaces are solid-liquid surfaces, and as such no doubt possess a high surface tension.1 The arrow C represents the sum of the radial components of the tension of both inner and outer surfaces of the membrane. Suppose now that a substance which lowers the surface tension of water, is added to some eggs in sea-water. By what is known as the Gibbs-Thomson Law it will tend to accumulate at the surfaces of the vitelline membrane. This will result in a lowering of the surface tension of the membrane.2 As a result equili- brium no longer exists, the force pushing outward is now stronger and the membrane is lifted away from the egg surface. Most of the egg proteins do not follow the membrane as it is lifted away, but remain in their original position. This is due to the fact that they diffuse much less readily than the salts, which can be thought of as pushing out the membrane. Around the mass of egg proteins a membrane is rapidly formed. As prev- iously pointed out, this "hyaline layer" is to be regarded as similar to the precipitation membranes formed on egg fragments. As a result of elevation, the vitelline membrane is no longer 1 The surface tension of a solid-liquid surface has never been accurately deter- mined, but there are various reasons for considering it the seat of an exceptionally high tension. For the similar case of a solid-gaseous surface Freundlich ("Kapil- larchemie," p. 90) following Quincke states that "die Oberflachenspannung fliissig- gasformig steigt allgemein mit sinkender Temperatur. Wenn nun die Flussigkeit stetig in einen amorph-festen Korper — eine Flussigkeit mit sehr groszer innerer Reibung— iibergeht, musz man auch annehmen, das die Oberflachenspannung bestehen bleibt, ja das sie zunehmend groszere Werte erhalt wenn sie sich auch wegen der groszen Zahigkeit nicht auszern kann." With the aid of a formula derived by Wilh. Ostwald, Freundlich calculates the tension of solid-liquid surface BaSC>4- water to be several thousand dynes per cm., an enormous value. In his wonderful treatment of capillarity Gibbs devotes a long section to the dis- cussion of solid-liquid surfaces (Gibbs, Collected Papers, I., 314-331), and derives various equations for them. In discussing the surfaces of the membranes, I have for the sake of simplicity not considered the presence of the jelly of chorion, which surrounds the egg. This is so diffuse a gel that it no doubt [has little if any effect. In fact, after its removal (by shaking), the eggs behave just as they did before. 2 Gibbs, loc. cit., p. 274: "Now the potential of a substance which forms a very small part of a homogeneous mass certainly increases, and probably very rapidly, as the proportion of that component is increased. (See (171) and (217).) The pressure, temperature, and the other potentials, will not be sensibly affected (see (98)). But the effect on the tension of this increase on the potential will be pro- portional to the surface-density, and will be to diminish the tension when the surface-density is positive (see (508))." The numbers refer to equations. When a substance accumulates at a surface, its surface density is by definition positive. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 167 in contact with the egg cytoplasm. It is, accordingly, now ex- posed on both of its faces to a solution which has the power of coagulating it. It might be expected that the process of gelatini- zation or coagulation be carried a step farther, and that the membrane become more rigid. This increased coagulation, if I may so speak of the process,1 results, as is typical for such cases, in an increase of refractive index, and the optical image of the membrane becomes more clearly defined. It may also result in an increased permeability of the membrane, for Quincke ('77) claims that precipitation membranes lose their semiper- meable properties on becoming rigid. On the basis of the above theory, it is evident that when the surface tension of the membrane drops below a certain limiting value, elevation -occurs. The determination of this value is at present impossible. It might be thought that the determi- nation of the surface tension of the various membrane-elevating solutions at their surface of contact with air, would throw some light upon the matter.2 But the lowering effect of a substance on a solid-liquid (or liquid-liquid) surface tension can not be measured by the effect the same substace produces on a gas- liquid surface. The chief reasons for this are: (i) Adsorption of added substances plays a very decided role in the case of the solid-liquid surface, different solids of course showing dif- ferent degrees of selective adsorption, (2) A solid-liquid surface is not subject to the action of evaporation, as is a gas-liquid surface. This is of especial importance if the substance which lowers surface tension is volatile. In discussing films like those of bubbles, Gibbs3 says: "But when a component which greatly diminishes the tension of the film although forming but a small fraction of its mass (therefore existing in excess at the surface), is volatile, the effect of evaporation and condensation may be considerable, even when the mean value of the potential for that component is the same in the film as in the surrounding atmosphere."4 Thus chloroform in water is quite effective 1 It might also be designated as "loss of water." 2 Czapek ('n) has thus endeavored to draw conclusions as to the surface tension of the plasma membrane of plant cells by measurements of air-liquid surface tensions. 3 L. c., p. 310. 4 A condition which would at least be approximated if the film were in equilibrium with its vapor in an enclosed chamber. 1 68 L. V. HEILBRUXX. for membrane elevation, but such a solution of chloroform has a surface tension against air just slightly below that of pure water.1 The great volatility of chloroform explains its inability to lower markedly the tension at the water-air surface; it is unable to accumulate in the surface film. But it can readily accumulate at a solid-liquid surface and here it no doubt pro- duces a marked lowering of surface tension. It follows that, although by a measurement of the air-water surface tension we can determine qualitatively whether a substance lowers the surface tension of the membrane or not, any truly quantitative measurements are impossible. In order to show qualitatively that a substance lowers the surface tension of the membrane, it is only necessary to make certain that its solution in water has a lower surface tension than the pure solvent. The theoretical basis for this assertion has already been pointed out. There is also another case to be considered. Some substances exert a liquefying effect upon the vitelline membrane. The liquefaction of a gel no doubt results in a considerable lowering of its surface tension.2 Ordinarily, however, the liquefying effect is a slow one, and in such cases, membrane elevation does not occur. For if the surface tension of the membrane is lowered only very slowly, the egg proteins have time to follow the membrane as it is pulled outwards. The result is an increase in egg diameter; the egg cytolyzes. Thus when the surface tension of the vitelline membrane is slowly lowered, cytolysis follows directly. On the other hand, when it is rapidly lowered membrane elevation takes place first and cytolysis follows after a short interval. After the vitelline membrane is elevated, it loses its semipermeability and the hyaline layer takes its place as the plasma-membrane of the egg cell (cf. p. 159). The same forces act on this membrane as were found to act on the vitelline membrane prior to its elevation. Hence when its surface tension is lowered, since the osmotic pressure within the egg is no longer completely counterbalanced, the hyaline layer tends to become pushed outward. But the hyaline layer appears to be more closely adherent to the egg proteins3 so that they are pulled out 1 Czapek ('n), p. 35. 2 Cf. note p. 166. 3 Probably because of a slight cortical coagulation. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHKM >< ,|..\ KM-,. 169 with it and the entire egg increases in diameter. According to this view cytolysis (when not directly an osmotic phenomenon) is in every case due to a lowering of the surface tension of the plasma membrane. This produces an influx of water into the cell. Various other explanations of cytolysis have been attempted. It has been regarded as due to the swelling of the egg proteins. This is an incorrect assump- tion, as I have already pointed out that the egg proteins, instead of becoming swollen, are coagulated in the cytolyzed egg (see pp. 195-198). It might also be considered as the result of a change in the plasma-membrane, of such a sort that this membrane would become readily permeable for salts though still impermeable for colloids. The unopposed osmotic pressure of the egg colloids would then push out the plasma membrane and the egg would increase its diameter. But such an explanation can not hold generally, for membrane-swelling although it produces cytolysis, does not render the plasma-membrane readily permeable to salts. In- deed in hypertonic salt solutions, eggs with swollen membranes shrink even more than those with unswollen membranes (see pp. 155-158). This is sufficient proof that the swollen membrane retains its semipermeability. In an earlier paper it was pointed out that all substances which had been described as producing membrane elevation (as well as some other newly discovered ones) did actually lower surface tension in aqueous solution. In a few substances, how- evei, this lowering could apparently only be a very slight one. These cases have been reinvestigated, and they have been found to involve membrane swelling rather than membrane elevation. When membrane swelling is rapid, it is possible that the lowered surface tension so produced should be followed by membrane elevation if the eggs are immediately returned to sea-water. One such case was indeed observed, but the observation was made at the close of the season, and further study of this point is necessary. D. Cortical Action of Heat and of Various Chemicals. Heat. — Although heat has been described as producing arti- ficial parthenogenesis in the sea-urchin egg (McClendon, !io), I know of no description of membrane elevation as a result of heat treatment. In several experiments, I was unsuccessful in producing membrane elevation in this fashion. In experiment a, eggs were dropped into sea-water which had been heated to 36.5 degrees, and were exposed I, 2, 3, 4 minutes. In experi- ment b, the eggs were dropped into sea-wrater heated to 35 de- 17° L. V. HEILBRUXX. grees, and were exposed 2, 4, 6, 8 minutes. In experiment c, eggs were dropped into sea-water heated to 37.5 degrees and kept at this temperature on a waterbath; the eggs were ex- posed 3^ minutes. In no case could any membrane elevation be observed, but the vitelline membrane did appear to be slightly swollen after the heat treatment. Alkalis. — If alkali is added to sea-water, the resultant pre- cipitation of magnesium salts tend to convert the sea-water into a solution containing little else than sodium salts. No doubt this fact alone is of significance. It is probable, however, that there is a more specific action of the alkali. As was shown in a previous discussion (p. 151), alkali apparently is able to liquefy the vitelline membrane. It is doubtful if any observer has actually obtained membrane elevation with NaOH or KOH. Certainly it does not occur when the eggs are left in the alkaline solution. Butyric Acid. — This famous method is one of the best for artificial membrane elevation. Loeb used it originally on Strongylocentrotus eggs, employing a concentration of 50 c.c. sea- water plus 2.8 c.c. N/io butyric acid and exposing the eggs 1^-3 minutes. In his recent book, he applies the method to Arbacia eggs reducing the concentration of acid slightly (50 c.c. sea-water plus 2.0 c.c. n/io butyric), but retaining the same time of exposure. He says that the eggs do not form a conspic- uous fertilization membrane, but only a "fine gelatinous layer which was not easily visible." This is evidently a membrane swelling and not an elevated membrane. For the benefit of workers in the field, I mention a slight modification in the method, by which true elevated membranes may be obtained for Arbacia eggs. Instead of 1^-3 minute exposures, a \ minute exposure was found very effective, and over ninety per cent, of elevated membrane could regularly be obtained if the eggs were well washed and in good condition. The concentration used was the original one of Loeb's, 50 c.c. sea-water plus 2.8 c.c. N/io butyric; slight variations are however immaterial. Although eggs exposed I minute often showed membrane elevation, no elevation was found to occur on longer exposure. Instead the membrane in every case appeared swollen, the amount of swelling increasing with the length of exposure. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. IJI Inorganic Salts. — That isotonic solutions of various salts cause membrane "formation" was discovered by R. Lillie ('10). The effectiveness of the various salts was found to correspond with their order in the lyotropic series, i. e., those salts which are most effective in producing swelling of protein gels (in alkaline solution) were also most effective in inducing membrane "formation." The reason for this correspondence becomes apparent, when we consider that the membrane "formation" produced by isotonic salt solutions is nothing other than a mem- brane swelling. The membranes resulting from salt treatment are sticky, they do not collapse in the presence of a colloid (cf. p. i 60). The concentration of a salt is an important factor in deter- mining whether swelling shall take place, and how pronounced the swelling shall be. Let us consider as an example the case of NaCl. Seven solutions of NaCl were prepared by adding distilled water to a 2.5 normal solution. Thus Solution A contained 50 c.c. 2\ M NaCl plus o c.c. distilled water. B 40 10 C 30 20 D 20 30 E 10 40 F 5 45 G 2\ 47-5 Eggs were placed in all these solutions at 3:30 P.M. By 3:39 eggs in A showed a membrane swollen to 2.51* in diameter. By 4:50 it had reached approximately $/j.. As for the eggs placed in B, the membrane swelled at least as rapidly as in the first case. Membranes on eggs in C and D also swelled rapidly, but in no case did eggs in E, F, G show any membrane swelling. Thus a certain concentration is necessary for swelling to occur. There appears to be at least two factors involved in this effect of concentration. In the first place, the swelling action of the salt no doubt increases with the concentration. But it is prob- able that the hypertonicity of the solution may also play a role.1 1 The explanation of such an effect might be as follows : When water is extracted from the cell, there is a tendency for vacuum production and consequent negative pressure (release of pressure) on the particles of the membrane. But we know L. V. HEILBRUNN. Sensitization with Strontium Chloride. — The process of sen- sitization was originally used by Loeb, later by Robertson, to induce the "formation" of a membrane in eggs exposed either to blood sera or to various tissue extracts, all dissoved in what was essentially a NaCl solution isotonic with sea-water. The method employed consists either of adding 3/8 M SrCl2 directly to the serum, or of placing the eggs first into the 3/8 M SrCl2 for several minutes, then into the serum. In the first paper of this series I urged that SrClo caused a precipitation of sul- phates and that much of the effect of sensitization was no doubt due to this action. In the summer of 1913, I was able to prove the truth of this statement. On July 31, 10 c.c. of 3/8 M SrCl2 was added to 90 c.c. of sea-water. The voluminous precipitate of SrSO4 was allowed to settle and filtered off, but the precipitate still continued to settle from the filtrate. When eggs were placed into this filtrate membrane swelling occurred, and this was followed by cytolysis. In another experiment, the eggs were placed first into 3/8 M SrCl2 and then into a 0.55 M NaCl solution. Used alone, the NaCl solution did not cause membrane swelling when two drops of a thick egg suspension were placed into 75 c.c. of it. But when several drops of egg suspension were placed into 3/8 M SrCl2, and then after five minutes, 2 drops of liquid containing eggs were taken from the SrCl2 solution and placed into the NaCl solution, membrane swelling could be observed to take place in the eggs so transferred. The vitelline membrane could be seen slowly to increase in thickness, so that after about half an hour a good proportion of the eggs were surrounded by a transparent outer layer which bore a close resemblance to an elevated membrane, except for the fact that the inner boundary of such a membrane was absent. The membranes produced as a result of the "sen- sitization" process did not collapse in the presence of a colloid. They were also sticky, and as a result the eggs tended to aggluti- nate. Such an agglutination of eggs with swollen membranes ' has no doubt led Robertson ('12) to the view that "fertilization and agglutination are similar phenomena." from Le Chatelier's theorem that whenever the pressure on a system in equilibrium is diminished, a change or reaction ensues which is accompanied by increase of volume. Hence the swelling. STUDIES IX ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 173 These facts enable us to interpret many of the results of Loeb and Robertson on a truly chemical basis, and without the aid of a hypothetical lysin. But there is also another factor to be considered in the analysis of the action of sera, and tissue ex- tracts. It is the fact that many proteins are probably able to effect a swelling of the vitelline membrane. Such an action is at least true for egg albumen. The eggs of 2 females were washed twice and pipetted into 100 c.c. of I per cent, egg al- bumen in sea-water. Before ten minutes had elapsed the mem- branes on most but not all of the eggs had swollen considerably. The swelling action of albumen seems to vary considerably, sometimes being almost negligible. It is, I think, to be regarded as a phenomenon akin to peptonization, the albumen playing the part of a protective colloid. KCN. — The action of potassium cyanide is rather difficult to explain on basis of the surface-tension theory. Although KCN alone, in the dilutions used, never caused elevation of the mem- brane, if the solution was made hypertonic the membrane did separate from the egg. Thus on July 31, 1914, membrane ele- vation did not occur in 25 c.c. of sea-water plus I c.c. of 1/20 per cent. KCN, but in a similar solution plus 4 c.c. 2.5 M NaCl, the vitelline membrane in practically every case became lifted away from the egg surface. How can this observation, which was repeated several times, be interpreted on the basis of the surface- tension theory? As is well known, KCN hydrolizes readily, so that KOH and HCN are always present in a solution of the cyanide. Probably HCN plays the most important part, for it is a gas, and hence its surface tension is practically zero. Thus it no doubt lowers the surface tension of the vitelline mem- brane (cf. p. 166). But membrane elevation does not result, because of another effect of the cyanide. In the first paper of this series, it was pointed out that even if a substance lowered surface tension, it would not produce membrane elevation if it increased too greatly the modulus of elasticity of the vitelline membrane, for in that case the toughened membrane would be incapable of stretching. There is evidence that KCN actually does increase the modulus of elasticity. In the next section it will be shown that the presence of KCN retards membrane swelling, and such anti-swelling action is a general characteristic of all 174 L- V- HEILBRUNN. substances which increase the modulus of elasticity.1 As a result of the stiffening action of KCN, the membrane resists elevation, for the elevating force is not sufficient to stretch it. But in a hypertonic solution, as the egg shrinks, no stretching is necessary to separate the vitelline membrane from the egg, and the mem- brane is accordingly pulled away from the egg surface. E. Inhibitors to Membrane Swelling. It has been found that swelling of the vitelline membrane may be retarded or even inhibited in the presence of certain substances. Up to the present only two such inhibiting sub- stances have been discovered. Of these, KCN inhibits the swelling of the membrane by salts, sea-urchin blood on the other hand inhibits acid swelling. But the inhibitor of salt action has no such effect on acid swelling, and sea-urchin blood in- stead of inhibiting swelling produced by salts, actually seems to favor it. These results, paradoxical as they appear at first sight, are really in direct line with recent findings in the field of colloid chemistry. There it has been shown that the salt and the acid swellings of gelatine must be essentially different processes, for the very salts which, by themselves, favor or even cause swelling, retard the swelling effect of an acid. Although membrane swelling usually occurs in "NaCl hyper- tonic sea-water,"2 in the presence of traces of KCN no such swelling will occur. This inhibiting effect of KCN has as yet not been found to be shared by any other substance, but so far only a few experiments in this direction have been made. Some of the experiments made during the summers of 1913 and 1914 are recorded below. August 29, 1913. To 200 c.c. of sea-water was added 32 c.c. of 2.5 M NaCl and the resultant "NaCl hypertonic sea-water" was divided into four 50 c.c. portions, A, B, C, D. To A was added 0.5 c.c. I per cent. KCN, to B 0.5 c.c. N/io NaOH, to C, 0.5 c.c. ether, and nothing was added to D, which several as a control. Eggs were then placed in A, B, C, D, and it was found that although membrane swelling very evidently took place in D, 1 Freundlich, "Kapillarchemie." p. 512. 2 As previously stated, I use this term to indicate a solution of 50 parts (by volume) of sea-water plus 8 parts of z\ M NaCl. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. IJ5 no such swelling occurred in A. The effect was not due to the alkaline reaction of KCN, for the swelling of egg membranes in B showed that NaOH had no retarding effect on the process. Likewise the ether present in C appeared to have no inhibiting effect. In other experiments KCN behaved similarly. In general it was found advisable to first place the eggs into sea-water which contained KCN and to add the 2.5 M NaCl later. Thus on August 29, 1914, it was found that some membrane swelling occurred when eggs were dropped into hypertonic sea-water to which KCN had already been added, but that this was entirely prevented when the eggs were first exposed to the KCN solution in sea-water, the 2.5 M NaCl being added later. August 2Q, 1914. Fingerbowls A, C, D, were used. Into A were placed 50 c.c. sea-water plus 8 drops of egg suspension, and to this were added 8 c.c. of 2.5 M NaCl. Fingerbowl C contained 50 c.c. sea-water plus i c.c. 1/5 per cent. KCN, to this were added at 10:30^ A.M. 8 c.c. 2.5 M NaCl, and at 10:31 A.M. 8 drops of egg suspension were dropped into the cyanide containing "NaCl hypertonic sea-water." Fingerbowl D contained 49 c.c. of sea-water plus i c.c. 1/5 per cent. KCN. Several drops of egg suspension were added to this solution of cyanide in sea-water, at 10:40 A.M. At 11:28 A.M. (48 minutes later), 8 c.c. of 2.5 M NaCl were added to D. On microscopical examination, it was found that pronounced membrane swelling had taken place in A (in the absence of KCN), swelling also occurred in C, but did not appear to be pronounced as that occurring in A. No membrane swelling at all could be observed in D, in which the eggs had been treated first with KCN before the concentrated NaCl solution had been added. The above experiment indicates that the action of KCN in inhibiting membrane swelling produced by NaCl, is not the result of a reaction between the cyanide and the salt, but is due to an effect of the cyanide on the membrane. For if only a reaction between salt and cyanide was involved, there could be no advantage in first subjecting the eggs to the action of the cyanide alone. Although KCN inhibits the membrane swelling effect of 176 L. V. HEILBRUNN. NaCl, it does not appear to have the slightest retarding effect on membrane swelling when this is produced by an acid. August 28, 1914. Fingerbowl A contained 50 c.c. sea-water plus 3 c.c. TV/io butyric acid. Fingerbowl B contained 49 c.c. sea-water plus 3 c.c. N/io butyric acid plus I c.c. i/io per cent. KCN. When eggs were added to A and B, membrane swelling occurred in both. September j, 1914. Stender dish A contained some eggs in 25 c.c. of sea-water. To this was added I c.c. 1/5 per cent. KCN at 10:52 A.M. Then 2 c.c. N/io butyric acid were added 29 minutes later (at 11:21). Stender dish B contained eggs in 25 c.c. of sea-water, 2 c.c. of N/io butyric acid were added at n :22 A.M. In both A and B, membrane swelling occurred and as a result the eggs in both cases stuck to each other and to the bottom of the dish. No difference could be observed between the two sets of eggs, and apparently the KCN has no effect on acid swelling of the membrane. KCN is thus capable of inhibiting membrane swelling by NaCl, but it has apparently no effect when the swelling is produced by butyric acid. On the other hand, sea-urchin blood was found to retard or inhibit acid swelling. June 22, 1914. A solution of butyric acid in sea-water was prepared by adding to 50 c.c. of sea-water, 2.5 c.c. of N/io butyric acid. Approximately 5 c.c. of the resulting solution were placed in each of two Syracuse watch-crystals (A and B}. To watch-crystal A were added 3 c.c. of filtered sea-urchin blood. (The blood was filtered after it had been allowed to "clot" by standing.) To watch-crystal B, 3 c.c. of sea-water were added. In B, membrane swelling and agglutination occurred, in A very little, if any, membrane swelling occurred, and there was no agglutination. The jelly was dissolved away from the eggs in A, but although the eggs were thus able to come into close con- tact, they would separate again, showing that they were not sticky, and that no membrane swelling had occurred. After a few hours, eggs in B had completely lost their color and ap- peared white to the naked eye, those in A appeared normal. July 7, 1914. The above experiment was repeated. In this case an acid solution was made up by adding 5 c.c. N/io butyric STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 177 acid to 50 c.c. of sea- water. Of this solution, 5 c.c. \vas put into each of t\vo Syracuse watch-crystals A and B. To A were added 5 c.c. of sea- water and five drops of egg suspension. To B were added 5 c.c. of filtered blood (from several cf s and 9 s) and then 5 drops of egg suspension. The result of the experiment was that in A the vitelline mem- branes swelled in practically every case. A count of a hundred eggs gave ninety-nine eggs with swollen membranes and the single exception was doubtful. On the contrary, there was practically no membrane swelling in B, which contained blood in addition to the acid. A count gave, of a hundred eggs ob- served, only three with swollen membranes. The inhibiting effect of blood upon acid swelling, unlike the effect of cyanide on salt swelling, may perhaps be the result of a direct action of the blood upon the acid. This is barely suggested by the fact that N/io HC1 produces a flocculent white precipitate when added to filtered sea-urchin blood. However in the above recorded experiments with butyric acid, no such precipitation could be observed. Instead of exhibiting a retarding effect upon membrane swelling by salts, sea-urchin blood seemed to favor the process. This favorable effect was much more pronounced in some cases than in others, and in several experiments it was not readily observed. On July 28, 1914, membrane swelling was found to be much more rapid and pronounced in a solution of 20 c.c. sea-water plus 5 c.c. filtered blood (from 9 s) plus 4 c.c. 2.5 M NaCl, than in a similar solution without blood, i. e., 25 c.c. sea- water plus 4 c.c. 2.5 M NaCl. It sometimes happens, and this appeared to be more frequent in 1914 than in 1913, that membrane swelling does not occur in "NaCl hyper tonic sea- water." In such cases, it was found possible in several instances to produce a membrane swelling by the addition of blood. Thus on July 30, 1914, although there was no swelling in "XaCl hypertonic sea- water," when eggs of the same lot were placed into 5 c.c. blood plus 20 c.c. sea- water plus 4 c.c. 2.5 M NaCl membrane swelling did occur. It might be reasoned that this action of blood is analogous to the cytolytic effect of sera foreign to the individual. But 1/8 L. V. HE1LBRUNN. in one case at least, blood of the same individual from which the eggs had been taken, was found to exert an accelerating effect upon membrane swelling in "NaCl hypertonic sea-water." If it be true that blood retards- acid swelling and favors salt swelling, this fact can be used in cases of doubt, to determine if a given type of membrane swelling is the result of the action of a salt or an acid. F. Cortical Changes at Fertilization. The central object of studies in artificial parthenogensis is to find an explanation of the processes occurring in normal ferti- lization. The fact that artificial membrane elevation is apparent- ly always the result of a lowered tension of the vitelline mem- brane has of course led to the view that the spermatozoon also produces a lowered surface tension. There are two conceivable ways in which this could happen. In the first place the sperm might carry a substance which lowers surface tension directly. This is improbable, in view of the fact that it has not been pos- sible to extract from sperm a membrane-elevating substance. It is more logical to suppose that the very act of penetrating on the part of the sperm lowers the tension. If the tension of a stretched thread be lowered at one point, instantaneously the tension throughout the thread is lowered. Similarly if the tension of a spherical stretched film or membrane be lowered at one point, there will be a lowering of tension in every point of the film, for in order that equilibrium be estab- lished, the tension in every part of the spherical film must be equal. This equalization of tension is probably a rapid process, especially when not merely a point, but an appreciable area of the surface has its tension lowered.1 Thus the penetration of the sperm almost immediately pro- duces a lowered tension in all parts of the vitelline membrane. In the sea-urchin egg, the sperm can not bore its way through the membrane mechanically, as it is not provided with a per- feratorium. It is therefore probable that the sperm has a solvent action on the membrane upon coming in contact with 1 However, in cases where a very thin film surrounds a large spherical mass as in air-bubbles the attainment of equilibrium between the parts of the film is much slower than the attainment of equilibrium between the film and the contiguous fluids. Cf. Gibbs, 1. c., p. 300. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESI-. 179 it. The partial liquefaction or swelling of the vitelline mem- brane at the point of sperm entrance can be conceived of as serving two functions: (i) it enables the sperm to enter, (2) it lowers the surface tension of the membrane and thus produces membrane elevation.1 That the sperm actually does produce a substance capable of causing membrane swelling, can be demonstrated. It is not possible to observe the swelling produced by a single sperm. But if the eggs are placed into very concentrated sperm suspen- sions, the vitelline membrane can be seen to swell all around the egg. Such concentrated suspensions are obtained by al- lowing the sea-urchins to shed their sperm. As is well known, the shedding reaction is aroused when the oral part of the shell is cut away. If the "dry" sperm be diluted only very slightly, an enormous sperm concentration can be obtained. When eggs are mixed with sperm suspensions of such high concentra- tion, each egg immediately becomes surrounded by a halo of wriggling sperm. Soon the vitelline membrane can be seen slowly to increase in thickness, it swells until it may reach a thickness of about 3 microns. That normal membrane elevation has not taken place can be shown by the fact that the swollen membranes thus produced do not collapse when the eggs are placed in a i per cent, or a 2 per cent, albumen solution (in sea- water). In a concentrated' sperm suspension, each point on the vitelline membrane is a point of attack on the part of the spermatozoa, and the entire membrane becomes swollen.2 The concentration of sperm necessary to produce a complete 1 It might be thought that puncture of the vitelline membrane, e. g., by a needle, should produce elevation. But this is not necessarily the case, for the hole pro- duced by a mechanical puncture of the membrane, if not immediately closed, would involve a loss of its semipermeable properties, and these on the basis of the theory (see pp. 165-166) are necessary for membrane elevation. A deep prick would also produce coagulation of the underlying cytoplasm, which would tend to prevent elevation. 2 It might be asked why elevation of the membrane does not follow swelling produced by concentrated sperm suspensions, since this no doubt results in a rapid lowering of surface tension. The answer is clear. In a previous paper (Heilbrunn, '13) it was pointed out that membrane elevation never occurred when the egg or its cortex was coagulated. Now in concentrated sperm suspensions, it can be shown that a profound coagulation does take place. This was demonstrated by a method which has been developed for revealing the presence of coagulation within the egg. For details of this method see p. 192. ISO L. V. HEILBRUNN. swelling of the membrane varies with the season. In the height of the season only quite concentrated sperm suspensions produce the phenomenon. But towards the end of August, as the season begins to wane, more and more dilute suspensions become ef- fective, until at the very close of the season, it is difficult to secure true membrane elevation at all. Probably the fluid emitted with the sperm is then charged with the substance which causes the swelling. In the preceding section it was shown that sea-urchin blood retards acid membrane swelling, but favors salt membrane swel- ling. This fact makes it possible to determine the general nature of the membrane swelling produced by sperm. One has only to observe the action of concentrated sperm suspensions in the presence of blood. If blood favors the swelling, this can be taken as evidence that the sperm action is similar to that of salts, if on the other hand it retards swelling, an acid is probably responsible. On July 7, 1914, 5 c.c. of sea-water plus 9 drops of egg suspension plus 5 drops of "dry" sperm were mixed in Syracuse watch-crystal A. Watch-crystal B contained 4 c.c. of blood (filtered from cfs and 9s) plus 9 drops of egg sus- pension plus approximately the same amount of sperm as did A. Both watch-crystals were shaken slightly to insure mixing. In A, the membranes swelled gradually; after 43 minutes they measured approximately 2p. In B, on the contrary, no membrane swelling could be observed. It might be objected that in the above experiment the blood had some effect which prevented intimate contact of sperm with egg. This objection is obviated by the following experi- ment. It was performed at the very close of the season, at a time when as previously pointed out, sperm suspensions have a much greater tendency to produce swelling. On September I, 1914, considerable difficulty was experienced in procuring sperm. Ten males were cut open and allowed to shed (several others had been rejected as being totally incapable). Of these ten, only two shed any sperm at all, and this was rather watery. Pre- liminary experiments showed that when 3 drops of this watery "dry" sperm were added to about 10 c.c. of sea-water and about 8 drops of the resulting suspension were again diluted with 10 c.c. of sea-water, a suspension was obtained which caused mem- STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. l8l brane swelling when 5 drops were added to 25 c.c. of sea-water, but only a moderate per cent, of fertilization when i drop was added to 25 c.c. of sea-water containing eggs. In the main experiment 3 drops of watery "dry" sperm were added to approx- imately 10 c.c. of sea-water, and the resulting suspension was the one used. Two Syracuse watch-crystals were employed. \Yntch-crystal A contained 4 c.c. of sea-water plus I c.c. of filtered blood (from 9s). Watch-crystal B contained 5 c.c. of sea-water and no blood. 8 drops of a dilute egg suspension were then added to A and to B (at n 115^ A.M.) and two min- utes later, 2 drops of the sterm suspension just mentioned, were added. At II :2O, eggs in B (i. e., in the absence of blood) all had membranes widely swollen all around. At 11:22, the eggs in A were examined. Most of the eggs showed not a trace of cortical change, but some showed membrane elevation. Short- ly after, a count was made of eggs in A. It was found that 23 showed no cortical change, I was doubtful, it may have had a swollen membrane, 9 evidently possessed elevated membranes. Thus the presence of blood prevents membrane swelling, and as one result of this prevention of excessive swelling, mem- brane elevation is possible, although without blood it could not have been produced. In this experiment, the anti-swelling effect of blood towards sperm suspension is conclusively demon- strated, for the blood evidently does not prevent access of sperm.1 The fact that the presence of blood inhibits membrane swelling in concentrated sperm suspensions, indicates that the swelling is produced by an acid. As is well known, all spermatozoa are abundantly provided with nucleic acid, and 'it is very probable that a nucleic acid or its derivative is responsible for the swelling. I have tried a number of times to watch the process of normal membrane elevation under high power, but with only scant success. The presence of a coverslip produces difficulties. If a drop of egg suspension is placed on each of two slides, and one 1 The further history of the eggs in this experiment is very interesting. On September 2, there are considerably fewer larvae in A (where blood was present) than in B. But whereas all of the larvae in A have well-marked arms, none of the larvae in B possess even the suggestion of arms. On September 3, the larvae in B are still perfectly armless, whereas those in A have the usual long arms of the pluteus stage l82 L. V. HEILBRUNN. drop is then covered by a coverslip with a drop of sperm sus- pension on its lower surface, and to the other drop is added a drop of the same sperm suspension, but no coverslip, a marked difference between the two preparations can be noted. In the absence of a coverslip, a much greater per cent, of eggs undergo membrane elevation. When the drop of sperm sus- pension is added to the eggs after the coverslip has been placed in position, practically no membrane elevation occurs. In these experiments, the coverslip was always supported by strips of paper or thin glass tubes, so that there could be no question of compressing the eggs. The effect of the coverslip is in part due to the action of the glass (or of substances diffusing out of it)1 on the spermatozoa. The sperm apparently congregate at the surface of the coverslip. But this is not believed to be the only effect, and some evidence that I possess, although not absolutely unimpeachable, tends to show that the pressure of the coverslip is also partly responsible for preventing elevation. However, further experiments on this point are necessary; I merely bring up the matter here in order to emphasize the difficulties in the way of direct obser- vation. Fol ('79) speaks of the great difficulty in observing fertilization in the sea-urchin egg. Pictet ('91) found no such difficulty, but I think that the cortical effect that he describes was not membrane elevation, but membrane swelling, which is not retarded by the presence of the coverslip.2 My observations, though admittedly incomplete, tend to show that the membrane is elevated from all sides of the egg at the same moment. It is possible that elevation does start at the point of sperm entrance as Ries ('09) for example claims, but if this part of the membrane does show any priority, it is only an exceedingly brief one. On the basis of Loeb's view that the sperm contains a lysin which produces membrane "formation" directly, we would have to suppose that this lysin diffuses around the egg surface in incredibly fast time. The morphology of sperm membrane elevation offers a severe obstacle to most theories which attempt 1 Possibly dissolved out by the alkali of sea-water. 2 Both Pictet and Fol subjected the eggs to slight compression. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 183 to account for the process, but it is readily understandable on the basis of the surface tension theory. The membrane swelling which occurs at the point of sperm entrance, causes a lowering of surface tension not only in the immediate vicinity, but everywhere on the egg surface. Hence the vitelline mem- brane is lifted from all parts of the egg at practically the same moment. G. The Significance of Cortical Change. Although it has never been proven that cortical change is absolutely essential before development can take place, it is generally admitted that membrane "formation" must precede any normal development. We- have seen that cortical change or membrane "formation" may involve either of two quite different processes. What then is the fundamental significance of cortical change, why is it necessary for normal development? I think that at least part of the answer is forthcoming. If an egg be watched under a micrometer scale, it will be noticed that shortly before the first cleavage the egg rapidly lengthens in the direction of its polar axes. In three minutes, an egg was in one case observed to increase its polar axis by 6.5/z, in another case by S/JL. (This fact is by no means new, all the pictures of cleavage show an increase in polar axis.) If the egg is surrounded by a stiff membrane, such a rapid change of form could scarcely be possible. But cortical change, whether it be membrane swelling or elevation, always results in the removal of this obstacle. The vitelline membrane is either rendered soft by swelling, or it is lifted away from the egg surface and its place taken by the no doubt less rigid hyaline layer. As a result, rapid changes irt egg form can occur. Moreover, as is well known, the hyaline layer is normally pulled in between the first two blastomeres during the cleavage process. The stiff vitelline membrane could scarcely act in this way. But either membrane swelling or membrane elevation would result in the egg being invested by a membrane which was not too rigid to be pulled in. Thus at least two processes which play a part in normal development would be greatly hindered if some kind of cortical change did not occur. 184 L. V. HEILBRUNN. Moreover when hypertonic solutions are used as reagents, the degree of rigidity of the plasma membrane becomes a factor of importance. As was shown previously (p. 158), exosmosis is favored by a cortical change which tends to soften the vitelline membrane. III. INTERNAL CHANGE. — THE PROBLEM OF SEGMENTATION A. Theories of Segmentation. The real problem in the study of artificial parthenogenesis is not the problem of cortical change, but is rather the analysis of the factors which produce initiation of development. How can we define initiation of development? Although it has been shown that after fertilization there is a quickening of various energetic processes in the sea-urchin egg, plausible as such a view no doubt is, no one has ever shown that all the recorded instances of artificial parthenogenesis involve an increased metabolism. ^ In practically every case, segmentation or mitosis has been regarded as the sole criterion of initiation of development. This is, I believe, wholly justifiable, as the act of segmentation is in itself a beginning of development. Of the various theories of artificial parthenogenesis, those of R. Lillie and of Loeb have in recent years met with the most favor. The former believes that the initial change is an increase in permeability of the plasma membrane. This is the direct cause of (i) mitosis, and (2) increased metabolism. In his theory of mitosis (R. Lillie 'u), now widely accepted as the least objectionable of any of the numerous theories of mitosis, he considers the hypothetical plasma membrane as charged by an electrical double layer; this charge is neutralized upon an increase in permeability. Such a neutralization of charge pro- duces, according to Lillie, a lower potential at the surface as compared with the interior, and the difference in potential thus produced brings about a number of internal changes, which lead finally to the formation of a mitotic spindle. I confess that I am unable to understand the fundamental basis of the theory. It appears to me that any drop in the potential at the surface of a body involves simultaneously an exactly equivalent drop of the potential at every point in the interior. This seems STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 185 a direct consequence of the textbook definition of potential at a point, as the amount of work done by the electric field on unit charge, when it is brought to that point from infinity. Moreover, even if we could accept some of the extiemely dubious evidence that has been offered in favor of increased permeability, no one has ever tried to show that all parthenogene- tic agents do cause increased permeability. In fact R. Lillie has at times assumed that hypertonic sea-water and that mag- nesium salts cause a decrease rather than an increase in per- meability, and yet hypertonic sea- water, even in the presence of magnesium salts, produces initiation of development. Of Loeb's ideas concerning the initiation of development only a bare outline is possible here. Recently, he is "inclined to be- lieve that in all cases in which an unfertilized egg has been caused to develop a typical or atypical membrane had been formed."1 This membrane "formation," which, as we have seen, Loeb regards as a swelling process, is the important initiative factor. It causes directly an increase of oxidations, but leaves the egg in a sickly condition, hence it is necessary to provide a corrective agent which may be either oxygen-containing hypertonic sea- water or the absence of oxygen. The theory of the necessity of the corrective factor need not concern us here, for we are primarily interested, not in the best method of obtaining arti- ficial parthenogenesis, but rather in the nature of the physical or chemical change which causes any initiation of development. Loeb believes this change to be an increase in oxidations. In support of this view he has accumulated a mass of data. He first showed that in his original method of using a hypertonic solution alone, artificial parthenogenesis could only be produced in the presence of oxygen. More recently he has measured egg oxidations and has confirmed Warburg's observation that mem- brane "formation" produces a great increase in oxidations. According to Loeb's measurements, any cytolytic change re- sults in a great increase of egg oxidations. B. The Action of Hypertonic Sea-water in the Presence of KCN. Loeb was originally led to adopt the oxidation theory, by the fact that either the addition of KCN, or the removal of oxy- 1 Loeb, '130, p. 223. 1 86 L. V. HEILBRUNN. gen by the passage of hydrogen, seemed to prevent the action of hypertonic solutions upon sea-urchin eggs. He reasoned from this that the hypertonic solutions produced an increase in oxidations. In 1913 and 1914, I performed a number of ex- periments with hypertonic solutions in the presence of KCN, or in an atmosphere of hydrogen. In this paper I shall, however, report only on the KCN experiments. The results obtained in an atmosphere of hydrogen are perhaps more interesting, but I do not feel as yet that every possible source of error has been eliminated. At present it is generally accepted by most physiologists that KCN suppresses cell oxidations.1 Loeb states that 2 c.c. 1/20 per cent. KCN to 50 c.c. of sea-water suffice for the purpose.2 In my experiments, a much higher concentration of the reagent was employed. On August 19, 1913, I added 0.5 c.c. of I per cent. KCN to every 58 c.c. of "NaCl hypertonic sea-water." (This addition of only 0.5 c.c. of more concentrated reagent had the advantage of not incurring as much dilution as the addition of 2 c.c. of almost pure solvent.) Eggs were exposed to this cyanide-con- taining "NaCl hypertonic sea- water," as well as to the normal "NaCl" hypertonic sea-water. The results appear in the fol- fowing table, in which the numerators of the fractions indicate the number of eggs observed to segment, the denominators the total number of eggs counted. Before return to sea-water, the eggs which had been exposed to KCN were washed twice by Lyon's test-tube method (Lyon '02), so that all trace of the poison might be removed. " NaCl Hypertonic Sea- Minutes Exposed. "NaCl Hypertonic Sea-water." water" plus KCN. 25 127/209 10/102 30 83/100 9/108 35 62/100 22/247 Of the eggs exposed 25 or 30 minutes to the cyanide-containing hypertonic solution, none went beyond the 2 (or 3)-celled stage, but of the 22 eggs which segmented after an exposure of 35 minutes, two became many-celled blastulae and may have gone 1 Personally I do not believe that the evidence in support of this view is con- clusive. 2 Loeb, '09, p. 55. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 187 farther, one reached a few-celled blastula stage. The control of unfertilized eggs in sea-water showed practically no seg- mentation, a count showed three out of more than five thousand. In the above experiment only about 30-40 per cent, of the eggs underwent membrane elevation. In other experiments, in which less concentrated solutions of KCN were employed, all cortical change was inhibited and the eggs did not segment, although nuclear division sometimes occurred. In order to obtain the best results, widely elevated membranes must be obtained. If the membranes were only lifted a short distance from the egg in the cyanide-containing hypertonic sea-water, on a return to normal sea-water the egg expanded until it again came into contact with the membrane (often it ruptured the membrane and an exovate was produced). The best results were obtained in an experiment of September 2, 1914. In this case, I c.c. of 1/5 per cent. KCN was added to 24.5 c.c. of sea- water containing eggs, at 11:45 A.M. At 11:45 !/6 A. M., 4 c.c. of 2.5 M NaCl were added, so that the eggs were then in "NaCl hypertonic sea-water" plus KCN. They were allowed to remain in this solution for 33 minutes, during which time it was noticed that ninety-seven per cent, underwent membrane elevation. At 12:18 P.M., some eggs in 4 c.c. of the solution were transferred to about 600 c.c. of sea-water in a tall graduate. At about 12:45 P. M., most of the sea-water was siphoned off from the graduate and fresh sea-water added. At 2:30 P.M., out of 233 eggs examined 22 were found to have segmented, but the count was evidently too low, as doubtful cases were rejected. In some instances, exovates simulated cleavage. Only those eggs in which a nucleus could be observed in each cell were counted. At 4:55 P.M., 50 eggs, out of 110 examined, were found to have segmented, but this count was likewise probably too low. At 9 P.M., hundreds of motile blastulae could be observed. (Of 337 eggs noted, 28 were motile larvae, of these 18 swam on the bottom, 10 on the top.) These experiments indicate that the action of the hypertonic solutions in initiating development is not due to an effect on the oxidative processes, for the hypertonic solution has the same action in the presence of a concentration of KCN, which ac- 188 L. V. HEILBRUNN. cording to Loeb is considerably above that sufficient to check oxidations. Loeb also found that the presence of KCN prevented degener- ative change in eggs exposed to hypertonic sea-water, and assumed that the KCN acted by retarding excessive oxidations. However on the basis of our knowledge concerning the anti- swelling effect of KCN (cf. p. 174), it is simpler to assume that the inhibition of membrane swelling is the prime cause in pre- venting disintegration by the hypertonic solution. The main support of the oxidation theory no doubt lies in the actual measurements of oxidations made by Warburg and Loeb. The method by which these measurements were made has been criticized in a note in Science (Heilbrunn '15). C. An Analysis of the Methods of Producing Segmentation in the Unfertilized Arbacia Egg. Hitherto in the study of artificial parthenogenesis, the general tendency has been to find new and different methods, rather than to find points of resemblance in the various means employed to produce segmentation in any one egg. The result has been that too much emphasis has been placed on the diversity and the unrelated character of the numerous parthenogenetic agents. As a matter of fact there are in general only two ways of pro- ducing segmentation in the Arbacia egg, the endosmotic method and the exosmotic method. Whenever a reagent lowers the surface tension of the plasma-membrane, endosmosis is the re- sult. The theoretical reason for this has already been considered (cf. p. 1 68). All those reagents which induce either true membrane elevation or membrane swelling are thus included in this cate- gory. In the first case the reagent produces a lowered surface- tension directly, in the second case, the swelling of the membrane results in a lowering of tension. Practically all of the reagents which have been used in artificial parthenogenesis produce either the one type of cortical change or the other. As a result, endos- mosis follows, unless the eggs are in a hypertonic solution. In the latter case, water is extracted from the cell, and exosmosis occurs. An apparent exception is found in the action of cold, which STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 189 has been stated to produce parthenogenesis in the sea-urchin egg (McClendon '10). Lowered temperatures should result in an increase, rather than a decrease of surface tension, nor can they be thought of as causing exosmosis. In the only experiment tried, I found that instead of producing artificial par- thenogenesis, lowered temperatures tended to retard the natural parthenogenesis which is usually manifested by the Arbacia egg. On June 25, the temperature of the aquarium in which the sea-urchins were kept, was 19.5°, the temperature of the sea-water as it emerged from the tap was 19°; the room temperature (at 4.45 P.M.) was 24.5°. At 10.40 A.M., a beaker containing eggs in a small amount of sea- water was placed in a large beaker, and the space intervening between the two beakers was filled with cracked ice. The beakers were then placed in the ice-box. At ii. oo A.M., the temperature of the sea-water surrounding the eggs was 3.5°, at 11.35 A.M. it was 1.5°, at 2.15 P.M. it was 0.5°, and at 4.15 P.M. it was 1.5°. Eggs were transferred from cold to normal sea-water after 3! hours (2.15 P.M.), 5 hours (3.40 P.M.), and 6\ hours (5.05 P.M.). We can refer to these three lots of eggs as lot A, lot B, lot C, respectively. Lot A when counted at 4.30 P.M. showed one doubtful case of segmentation, out of 100 eggs observed. Lot B at 5.30 P.M., out of 1,000 eggs counted, showed 3 eggs apparently cleaving irregularly, and i doubtful case. At best 4/1000. Lot C at 8.55 P.M. showed 9/1000 cleavages. The control of untreated eggs (from the same female) at 4.40 P.M. showed, of 1,100 eggs counted, 4 eggs cleaving irregularly (i a 7-celled stage) and 5 with attempted or incomplete cleavages. At 9.05 P.M. however, the control showed a much higher count. Of 526 eggs counted, 36 showed irregular divisions. Thus of the eggs counted at about 9.00 o'clock, those which had been exposed to cold for 6| hours showed less than i per cent, of segmenting eggs, whereas the control showed over 6 per cent. Thus the cold evidently retarded the process of natural parthenogenesis. Of the two general methods of obtaining parthenogenesis, the exosmotic method yields the better results. Usually the endosmotic method produces only a small per cent, of segmenting eggs. But this is probably due to the specific poisoning action of the substances used in lowering surface tension, for when the sea-water is simply diluted, or when a harmless substance like egg albumen is used, much higher per cents, of dividing eggs are obtained. For example, when eggs were subjected to the action of a i per cent, solution of egg albumen (Kahlbaum) in sea-water, more than half of them divided, as shown in the accompanying table. In such a solution, the eggs were observed to undergo membrane swelling (see p. 173) and this was followed bv endosmosis as shown bv the increased diameter. L. V. HEILBRUNN. Length of Exposure to i Per Cent. Egg Albumen. Segmentations. 30 minutes 55/ioo 66 48/100 118 SO/TOO 233 6/100 The results gained with the endosmotic method used alone, are never as good as those which can be obtained with the ex- osmotic method. Neither are the per cents, of segmentation as high, nor is the degree of development attained as great. In spite of the fact that Loeb ascribes to hypertonic solutions a mere correcting effect, it is I think a noteworthy fact that no method of artificial parthenogenesis yet tried on the Arbacia egg, is truly effective, unless at some stage of the process it requires a hypertonic solution. In connection with this point, I made a number of experiments to test the butyric acid-KCN method which Loeb found so effective for Stronglocentrotus. I was at the time convinced that the method was essentially the same as Delage's acid and alkali method (Delage '07), and I tried to find if NaOH could not be substituted for the KCN, in other words if the correcting action of the latter was not due solely to its alkalinity. In no case, however, was I able to get any results either with KCN or NaOH as a "correcting agent" after butyric acid treatment. Indeed recently Loeb ('136) has pointed out that the method is not suited for the Arbacia egg. The question now arises if the two methods of producing segmentation, the one endosmotic, the other exosmotic, have anything in common. As a result of my experiments, I find that both methods produce a gelatinization or coagulation within the egg.1 Most biologists believe that the mitotic spindle is a con- densation or coagulation product, and there is excellent support for this view. Hence any initiation of development must soon 1 In the test-tube, gelatinization and coagulation of proteins are apparently quite different phenomena, the former converting the entire mass into a jelly, the latter resulting in a separation of a precipitate. But in the small field of action of a sea-urchin egg, it would not be so easy to distinguish between the two, for the entire egg is smaller than a single flake of the usual precipitate. Moreover, even in the test-tube coagulation is usually preceded by a stage strictly comparable to gelatinization; the entire mass becomes opalescent and assumes a greatly increased viscosity; only later does the precipitate appear. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 19 1 lead to some coagulation before mitosis can be accomplished. At present all students of artificial parthenogenesis, if they con- sider this coagulation at all, regard it as a secondary result. Thus, some think an increase of oxidations is first produced by all parthenogenetic agents, and that the increased oxidation involves changes which produce coagulation. Others think of the prime cause as an increase of permeability. But it is also possible to believe that the primary effect of all the partheno- genetic reagents is a coagulation effect. This view has had as its adherents, at one time or another, some of the foremost students of artificial parthenogenesis. In his earliest papers on the subject, Loeb occasionally leaned toward coagulation as a possible primary effect, but he soon abandoned the idea to become its vigorous opponent. Delage for many years main- tained that artificial parthenogenesis is the result of a coagu- lation followed by a liquefaction, he considered membrane elevation as one evidence of such a coagulation. At present, however, (Delage and Goldschmidt '13), he favors the Lillie theory of increased permeability as affording a more probable explanation of the facts. Possibly the most vigorous and scientific attempt to support the coagulation theory was that of Fischer and Ostwald ('05). These workers argued from a theo- retical standpoint that all parthenogenetic agents are of such a nature as to produce coagulation. Later Ostwald ('07) retreated from this view and admitted that coagulation might be only secondarily produced as a result of increased oxidation. That all parthenogenetic agents cause coagulation, was denied by Loeb.1 He pointed out that benzol, toluol, and saponin are not protein coagulants. Since then the theory of Fischer and Ostwald had had no one to defend it. The view that I would maintain is that the only physico- chemical effect which all parthenogenetic agents possess in common is the production of a gelatinization or coagulation within the egg. Hence I regard this gelatinization (or coagu- lation) as the direct cause of the initiation of development. Leaving theory aside, it is possible to demonstrate that all parthenogenetic agents actually do produce gelatinization or 1 Loeb, '09, p. 217. 192 L. V. HEILBRUNN. coagulation within the egg. The distinguishing feature of a gel as opposed to a sol is its greater viscosity. All other macroscopic differences depend upon this. Indeed, according to Freund- lich, the viscosity of a colloidal solution may be taken as a measure of its tendency to gelatinize ("Gelatinierungsbestreben").1 The viscosity of the Arbacia egg protoplasm was regarded as an index of the state of aggregation of its constituents. There are two general methods of measuring viscosity. One can either measure the rate of flow of a fluid, or one can study the movement of particles through the fluid. Both of these methods were used in studying the viscosity of the Arbacia cytoplasm. It is very easy to observe the rate of flow of the egg cytoplasm. One has only to exert pressure on the coverslip and the vitelline membrane soon bursts, allowing the egg con- tents to flow out. With this method, only great changes in viscosity can be noted, but gelatinization always does produce a very great increase in viscosity, so that the method usually suffices. The pressure can be applied by pushing on the cover- slip with the point of a dissecting needle, or if greater accuracy is desired, a square piece of glass (broken from a slide) can be dropped from a known distance. This method of observing changes in viscosity is not entirely above criticism; for cortical changes, produced by the reagents used, might have an effect on the size of the aperture through which the cytoplasm flows. The second method is much more exact and reliable. It involves a study of the movement of granules through the cytoplasm. The force which must be exerted to push the granules through the cytoplasm is a measu e of the viscosity of the latter. Centrifugal force is of necessity used, and a hand centrifuge does very nicely.2 Into one tube of the centrifuge are placed eggs which have been treated in various ways; into the other, normal eggs. After the centrifuging has been accom- plished, a microscopic examination reveals any differences which 1 Freundlich, '09, p. 416. Freundlich and Ishizaka, '13. 2 A Bausch and Lomb instrument was used in my experiments, and the eggs were placed into the small glass tubes of the haematocrit attachment. New tubes were used in each experiment so that all danger of contamination was avoided. One turn of the high-speed handle involved 130 revolutions of the tubes. The distance between the end of each tube and the axis was approximately 7^ cm. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 1 93 may exist between norma' and treated eggs. When normal unfertilized Arbacia eggs are centrifuged vigorously, the pro- toplasmic materials rapidly become separated into four layers or zones. Lyon ('07) has given an excellent description of the appearance of these zones and the reader is referred to his paper for detai s. The pigment-bearing granules come to be all massed at one pole, in the pigment zone; next to this is a zone of granular material, then a hyaline zone, and at the pole opposite the pig- ment zone is a small dense accumulation of substance which because of its color is known as the gray cap. The egg nucleus lies in the hyaline zone, directly beneath the gray cap. When an egg shows all these zones, I shall refer to it as "stratified." As the viscosity of the protoplasm increases, stratification be- comes more and more difficult; in a thoroughly coagulated egg no stratification is possible. Hypertonic solutions produce a very noticeable coagulative change in the sea-urchin egg. In 1913 a few preliminary ex- periments were performed in which the eggs were pressed out of shape by pushing down on the coverslip with a dissecting needle. It was found that "Nad hypertonic sea-water >: produced a marked increase in the viscosity of the cytoplasm. Such a solution causes swelling of the vitelline membrane. This swelling is apparently absent if a freshly prepared 0.49 M MgCl2 solution is used. Eggs treated with this solution became very much more viscous than they had been previously. Whereas the normal unfertilized eggs shot out their contents rapidly if subjected to a slight pressure, eggs which had been immersed in MgCl2 for 85 minutes could be subjected to considerable pres- sure without losing their circular outline. They could indeed be flattened out into a thin "pancake." More accurate data were obtained with the centrifuging method. On July 23, 1914, some eggs were placed into 50 c.c. of sea-water plus 8 c.c. of 2.5 M NaCl at 2:2 if P.M. At 2:44 P.M., these eggs were placed into one tube of the centrifuge, and into the other tube were placed some normal untreated eggs of the same female. At 2:45 P.M., after a few preliminary turns, the tubes were revolved for 28 seconds at a rate of 162 revolutions per second. The eggs were then examined. The L- v- HEILBRUNN. normal eggs showed the typical zones of centrifuged eggs. The pigment zone extended for about one fourth of the egg diameter along the axis of stratification. The contrast between normal eggs and those which had been treated with the hypertonic solution was very marked. The latter as a rule showed no stratification whatsoever, although some eggs showed just the beginnings of such a process, the pigment granules being slightly more abundant at one pole of the egg than at the pole opposite to it. The experiment was repeated on July 24. In this case un- fertilized eggs were subjected to "NaCl hypertonic sea-water" at 10:29 A.M., and about 25 minutes later they were placed into one tube of the centrifuge. Into the other tube, normal eggs were placed. Then (at 10:55^ A.M.) the tubes were revolved for 19 seconds at the rate of 171 revolutions per second. Upon examination, the normal eggs showed complete strati- fication under low power of the microscope. High power examina- tion showed that a few pigment granules had not reached the pigment zone, but had lingered near the equator of the eggs. The eggs treated with hypertonic sea-water showed no strati- fication. In some eggs one pole was slightly paler than the other, and fewer pigment-bearing granules could be found at this pole. But even in these eggs, the pigment was scattered throughout every part of the cytoplasm. At 11:29 the eggs in hypertonic sea-water were centrifuged again and compared with normal eggs given the same treatment. Longer and more vigorous centrifuging was resorted to, and the tubes of the centri- fuge were revolved 186 times per second for 35 seconds. The normal eggs now appeared perfectly stratified; they were often elongated as a result of the treatment. The eggs in hypertonic sea- water usually showed only the beginnings of stratification, a tendency for the pigment to be massed toward one pole. In a few eggs, however, the pigment was practically limited to one half of the egg. These experiments show a striking increase in cytoplasmic viscosity after eggs have been exposed to hypertonic solutions. This could be noted both by observation of the flow of the cyto- plasm itself, as well as by more careful observations of the STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 1 95 movements of granules through the cytoplasm. The only explanation of such a marked increase in viscosity is that gela- tinization or coagulation has taken place within the egg. Un- doubtedly exosmosis causes some constituent of the cytoplasm to change from sol to gel. Endosmosis was also found to cause coagulation, either when it resulted from a dilution of the outer medium, or when it was the result of a lowered surface tension of the vitelline or plasma membrane. This could be demonstrated either by observing the cytoplasmic flow from compressed eggs, or by noting the granular movements in centrifuged eggs. Numerous experi- ments were made with distilled water. In an experiment of June 30, 1914, a square piece of glass which had been broken from a slide was used to compress the eggs. It weighed 1.31 grams. The piece of glass was held between the thumb and forefinger, so that its one edge rested on the slide just to one side of the coverslip which covered the eggs under observation. At the desired time, the glass piece was dropped and the com- pressed eggs could then be immediately observed. Normal eggs were deprived of their jelly by shaking them 6 or 8 times in a test-tube, and were then subjected to the pressure of the piece of glass as just described. The eggs all ruptured, the contents flowing out for a distance of about 60/1. Eggs (from the same female) were then dropped into distilled water at 10:28 A.M. At io:28f any jelly which may have remained around them was removed by shaking the eggs. At 10:29^ the eggs were subjected to the pressure of the same piece of glass which had been used in the case of the normal eggs. They resisted the pressure and remained circular or nearly circular in outline. In the eggs observed, membrane elevation had not taken place. The greater resistance to pressure of the eggs treated writh distilled water is an indication of their increased viscosity. The eggs tended to remain spherical, they showed a definite elasticity. Similar observations showing increased viscosity after treatment with distilled water, were made a number of times both in 1913 and 1914. On July 25, 1914, at 3:59 P.M., 7 drops of an egg suspension were dropped into 15 c.c. of distilled water. The eggs in the 196 L. V. HEILBRUNX. distilled water were then hastily pipetted into one tube of the centrifuge, the other tube contained untreated eggs. The centrifuge was started at 4:00^ P.M., and for 25 seconds the tubes were revolved at a rate of 156 revolutions per second. The eggs in distilled water were examined as soon as possible (about half a minute later). They showed not a sign of strati- fication. Most of the pigment had been lost as a result of the action of the distilled water, but high power examination showed that the pigment-bearing granules still retained some pigment. These granules were scattered all through the cell. On the other hand the normal eggs showed the typical stratification. The viscosity of the cytoplasm had therefore increased enor- mously in the eggs treated with distilled wrater, so that the granules were prevented from wandering through it. In another experiment (July 22, 1914) the eggs wrere transferred back to sea-water before being centrifuged. At 3:51 P.M., 5 drops of egg suspension were added to 15 c.c. of distilled water. At 3:55 P.M., some of these eggs were removed from the distilled water and placed into sea-water again. The eggs thus trans- ferred were the ones studied; they were placed into one tube of the centrifuge, normal eggs occupying the other. Beginning at 3:59^ P.M., the tubes were revolved 165 times per second for 30 seconds. Upon examination it was found that whereas the normal eggs were completely stratified, not a sign of stratification could be observed in the eggs which had been exposed to distilled water. Evidently endosmosis, following immersion in distilled water, leads to a gelatinization or coagulation in the cytoplasm. This effect is also produced when endosmosis follows a lowered surface tension of the plasma membrane. A drop of egg sus- pension was stirred up with a drop of chloroform, and the eggs rapidly increased in diameter. When subjected to the pressure of a piece of glass broken from a slide, the eggs flattened out but remained perfectly circular in outline. The same result was obtained if toluol was used instead of chloroform. The eggs under pressure behave like eggs which have been coagulated by some typical coagulant, e. g., HgCl2. On the other hand, when the normal eggs were subjected to the pressure of the same piece of glass, the cytoplasm flowed out a considerable distance. The STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. results obtained with the centrifuging method are still more convincing. At 10:02^ A.M. (July 23, 1914), 2.5 c.c. of toluol were added to 2.5 c.c. of sea- water containing eggs, and the mixture was stirred thoroughly with a glass rod (in a Syracuse watch-crystal). At 10:07^ A.M., 7 drops of the egg-containing liquid were transferred back to normal sea-water. Into one tube of the centrifuge were placed normal eggs, the other contained eggs which had been treated with toluol for five minutes. The eggs were then centrifuged for 30 seconds at a rate of 152 revolutions per second. Upon examination, the normal eggs appeared perfectly stratified, whereas those which had been exposed to toluol showed not a trace of stratification. As a result of the toluol treatment the eggs increased their diameter about 2/4. The experiment was repeated with identical results on July 25, I c.c. of toluol being added to 3 c.c. of sea-water. In this case the eggs were exposed 5 minutes, and were then centrifuged 23 seconds at an average of 158 revolutions per second. Saponin was also found to produce a coagulation within the egg. At 5:35 P.M. (July 23), 7 drops of an egg suspension were placed in about 15 c.c. of 0.2 per cent, saponin solution (in sea-water). -At 5 140 P.M., the eggs were returned to normal sea- water. The eggs were then subjected to an exceptionally long centrifuging process. Beginning at 5:47 P.M., for 50 seconds they were revolved at an average of 166.5 revolutions per second. Normal eggs subjected to this centrifuging process were of course completely stratified. Of the eggs treated with saponin, only about 15-20 per cent, showed any signs of strati- fication, the remaining eggs were all totally unstratified. In the experiments with saponin and toluol, the reagent pro- duces a lowering of surface tension directly. With some re- agents, membrane swelling occurs first, and the lowered surface tension thus produced results in endosmosis. In this case also, coagulation follows an increase in egg volume. On August 4, 1914, eggs were washed in 0.55 M NaCl solution and were then dropped into 0.55 M NaCl plus 1.5 c.c. N/io NaOH at 10:59^ A.M. These eggs were then placed into one tube of the centri- fuge, and into the other were placed eggs which had been im- 198 L. V. HEILBRUNN. mersed in 25 c.c. of 0.55 M NaCl at 10:58 A.M. Beginning at 11:165 A.M., the eggs were centrifuged for 17 seconds at the rate of 153 revolutions per second. When examined, the eggs in NaCl alone showed stratification, although the boundary between the various zones was not a sharp one. Gray cap, hyaline zone, granular zone, were distinguishable, but the pig- ment was not entirely restricted to^the pigment zone. On the other hand, the eggs in the alkaline NaCl solution showed not a vestige of stratification, in every case the pigment-bearing granules were evenly distributed throughout the cell. In these eggs exposed to an alkaline NaCl solution, membrane swelling had taken place. Similarly when membrane swelling was in- duced by i per cent, albumen, coagulation followed the endos- mosis thus produced. On August 4, 1914, at 10:40 A.M., some eggs were placed in a filtered I per cent, solution of egg albumen solution in sea-water. After about 50 minutes, some eggs in the albumen solution were put into one tube of the centrifuge, normal eggs were placed in the other. The eggs were then centrifuged at the rate of 143/evolutions persecond for2O seconds. The normal eggs all showed stratification, although the various zones were not sharply marked off from each other. Of the eggs in the albumen solution only a few were examined. Of these ten showed not a trace of stratification, one egg showed stratification, but on examination it was found that its membrane had remained unswollen. Pressure experiments also showed a great increase in viscosity after membrane swelling. Sodium iodide and I per cent, albumen were used in these experiments. It is evident that botfy Jhe reagents which cause exosmosis and those which produce endosmosis, cause gelatinization or coagulation of some substance fn ttye Arbacia egg. Thus all parthenogenetic agents produce such ^coagulative change.1 The experiments indicate that the coagulation occurs just as 1 I have omitted mention of radium and ultra-violet rays as parthenogenetic agents. I have not worked with either of these methods, and so can only offer a theoretical interpretation. Judging from Loeb's description (Science, N.S., XL., 680 (1914)), ultra-violet rays produce membrane swelling and thus they no doubt induce endosmosis. Moreover both radium and ultra-violet rays are protein coagulants, according to Dreyer and Hanssen, C. R. Acad. Sci., CXLV., 234 (1907). Hardy has also decribed gelatinization of a globulin solution as a result of radium radiation. Proc. Camb. Phil. Soc., XII., 201 (1903). STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 199 soon as water enters or leaves the cell. It is probable therefore that the effect is primary, and not the result of intermediate changes. In normal fertilization, the sperm likewise produces coagula- tive changes in the cytoplasm. Albrecht in 1898 showed that there was an increase in viscosity after fertilization. He drew his conclusions from observations on compressed eggs. I have repeated Albrecht's observations a number of times. Much more striking demonstration of gelatinization or coagulation after fertilization is afforded by the centrifuge method. On July 22, 1914, eggs from a single female were washed twice and divided into two lots. At 10:00 P.M., half of the eggs were fertilized. One tube of the centrifuge was then filled with fer- tilized eggs, the other contained unfertilized eggs. At 10:10 P.M., the tubes were revolved for 18 seconds at an average speed of 180.5 revolutions per second. The eggs were then examined immediately. The unfertilized eggs were typically stratified. On the other hand, the eggs which had been ferti- lized were not at all stratified. Out of hundreds examined, only one showed stratification and that was peculiar in lacking an elevated membrane. Probably it had escaped fertilization. The coagulative change begins to be apparent very soon after fertilization. In one case the beginnings of the process could be observed 2\ minutes after insemination. At 8:24 P.M. (July 26) some eggs wrere fertilized in a small volume of sea-water and were shaken about to insure rapid sperm contact. At 8:26^ P.M., fertilized and unfertilized eggs were centrifuged at the same time (in separate tubes). For 23 seconds an average speed of 141 revolutions per second was maintained. Upon examination the fertilized eggs showed some evidences of strati- fication. The gray cap was becoming evident in some. But most of the fertilized eggs showed little more than a tendency for the pigment to mass at one pole. The unfertilized eggs were clearly more stratified. They showed the gray cap plainly in all cases, and a hyaline zone was also recognizable in them. Thus only 2\ minutes after insemination, the sperm had already begun to produce coagulative changes in the cytoplasm. From these experiments, I have been led to conclude that 2OO L. V. HEILBRUNN. initiation of development, either artificially produced or the result of fertilization, always involves a gelatinization or coagu- lation with the egg. This coagulative change is evoked before the egg interior shows any other signs of the approach of develop- ment. The coagulating substance (or substances) is evidently very ready to coagulate. The ensemble of conditions within the egg is no doubt responsible for this unstable state. Only a slight change in salt concentration is then sufficient to bring about coagulation. The fact that both increase and decrease of salt concentration are effective, suggests that the protein involved is of the globulin type.1 The mitotic spindle probably arises as a direct result of the coagulative change. The actual explanation of how this occurs is not truly a problem of biology but a problem of colloid chemis- try, for it has been shown (Fischer, '99) that coagulation of inanimate proteins can produce structures identical in appear- ance with the mitotic spindle. IV. SUMMARY. 1. The unfertilized Arbacia egg consists essentially of fluid proteins, surrounded by a stiff membrane (the vitelline mem- brane), which is the plasma membrane of the egg cell. This membrane is a protein gel with little or no admixture of lipoids. 2. There are two types of cortical change, membrane swelling and membrane elevation. In the former, the membrane ab- sorbs water and increases in thickness; in the latter, the normal process, it becomes lifted away from the egg surface. 3. The vitelline membrane only loses its semipermeable proper- ties several minutes after elevation, and the increase of permea- bility that then ensues is best regarded as a result rather than as a cause of the process. 4. Artificial membrane elevation is produced only by sub- stances which lower the surface tension of the vitelline mem- brane. This is explainable on the basis of a theory that considers the various forces at play on the membrane. After its surface tension is lowered, the forces exerted outward are stronger than 1 If the protein referred to here is, as seems most likely, the substance which forms the spindle fibers, the fact that it shows the properties of a globulin becomes of significance. For globulins are the proteins most closely associated with con- tractile processes, muscles consisting almost entirely of them. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 2OI those balancing them, and the membrane is as a result pushed away from the egg. 5. Cytolysis is not due to a simple swelling of egg proteins for these can be shown to coagulate rather than to swell, it is due to a continuation of the same process as that which results first in membrane elevation. 6. The sperm produces membrane elevation by lowering the surface tension of the vitelline membrane. This it accomplishes by causing the membrane to swell at the point where it strikes the egg. 7. Experimental evidence is presented to show that this membrane swelling produced by the sperm is due to the action of an acid. 8. The fact that potassium cyanide does not prevent initiation of development by hypertonic sea-water, is taken as evidence against the oxidation theory of artificial parthenogenesis. 9. All initiation of development involves the gelatinization or coagulation of some substance within the egg. This coagu- lative change can be demonstrated to take place before the egg interior shows any other signs of the approach of development. The above summary covers only the more important points of the paper. I desire to thank Professor F. R. Lillie, and also Professors Child and Tower, for their kind criticism of the manuscript. To Professor Lillie I am also indebted for the use of a table at the Woods Hole Marine Biological Laboratory, where all of the experimental work was done. V. REFERENCES. Albrecht, E. '98 Untersuchungen zur Struktur des Seeigeleies. Sitzungsber. d. Ges. f. Morph. u. Physiol. in Miinchen, 1898, pp. 133-141. Czapek. 'n Ueber cine Methode zur direkten Bestimmung der Oberflachenspannung der Plasmahaut von Pflanzenzellen, Jena, 1911, G. Fischer. Delage, Y. '07 L'oxygene, la pression osmotique, Jes acides et les alcalis dans la partheno- genese experimentale. C. R. Acad. Sci. Paris, T. 145, pp. 218-224. Delage, Y. and Goldschmidt, M. '13 La parthenogenese naturelle et experimentale, Paris, 1913. 202 L. V. HEILBRUNN. Fischer, A. '99 Fixirung, Farbung und Bau des Protoplasmas, Jena, 1899, G. Fischer. Fischer, M. H. '10 Oedema. New York, Wiley. '12 A Further Response to Some Criticisms of the Colloid-chemical Theory of Water Absorption by Protoplasm. Journ. Amer. Med. Assn., Vol. 59, pp. 1429-1433. Fischer, M. H. and Ostwald, Wolfgang. '05 Zur physikalisch-chemischen Theorie der Befruchtung. Arch. f. d. ges. Physiol., Bd. 106, pp. 229-266. Fol, H. '79 Recherches sur la fecondation et le commencement de 1'henogenie chez divers animaux. Mem. d. 1. Soc. de Phys. et d'Hist. Nat. de Geneve, T. 26. Freundlich, H. '09 Kapillarchemie, Leipzig, 1909. Freundlich, H., and Ishizaka, N. '13 The Velocity of Coagulation of Aluminium Hydroxide Sols as Measured by Changes in the Viscosity. Trans. Faraday Society, Vol. 9, pp. 66-79. Gibbs, J. Willard. '7S-'78 On the Equilibrium of Heterogeneous Substances. Republished in the Scientific Papers of J. Willard Gibbs, London and New York, 1906. Heilbrunn, L. V. '13 Studies in Artificial Parthenogenesis — I. Membrane Elevation in the Arbacia Egg. BIOL. BULL., Vol. 24, pp. 345-361. '15 The Measurement of Oxidations in the Sea-urchin Egg. Science (in press). Herxheimer, G. Mikroskopische Technik, in Abderhalden's Handbuch der biochemischen Arbeitsmethoden, Bd. VII., pp. 632-714 (see pp. 679-680). Kite, G. L. '13 Studies on the Physical Properties of Protoplasm. I. The Physical Prop- erties of the Protoplasm of Certain Animal and Plant Cells. Amer. Journ. Physiol., Vol. 32, pp. 146-164. Lepeschkin, W. W. 'n Zur Kenntnis der chemischen Zusammensetzung der Plasmamembran. Ber. d. deutsch. bot. Ges., Bd. 29, pp. 247-261. Lillie, Ralph S. '10 The Physiology of Cell Division — II. The Action of Isotonic Solutions of Neutral Salts on Unfertilized Eggs of Asterias and Arbacia. Amer. Journ. Physiol., Vol. 26, pp. 106-133. 'n The Physiology of Cell Division — IV. The Action of Salt Solutions Followed by Hypertonic Sea-water on Unfertilized Sea-urchin Eggs and the Role of Membranes in Mitosis. Journ. Morph., Vol. 22, pp. 695-730. Loeb, Jacques. '08 Ueber den Mechanismus der Agglutination. Kolloid-Zeitsch., Bd. 3, pp. H3-II4- '09 Die chemische Entwicklungserregung des tierischen Eies, Berlin, 1909, Springer. 'i3a Artificial Parthenogenesis and Fertilization. Chicago, 1913, University of Chicago Press. STUDIES IN ARTIFICIAL PARTHENOGENESIS. 2O3 'i3b Reversibility in Artificial Parthenogenesis. Science, N.S., Vol. 38, pp. 749-751. Loewe, S. '13 Membran and Narkose. VVeitere Beitrage zu einer Kolloidchemischen Theorie der Narkose. Biochem. Zeitschr., Bd. 57, pp. 161-260. Lyon, E. P. '02 Effects of Potassium Cyanide and of Lack of Oxygen upon the Fertilized Eggs and the Embryos of the Sea-urchin (Arbacia punctulala). Amer. Journ. Physiol., Vol. 7, pp. 56-75. '07 Results of Centrifugalizing Eggs. Arch. f. Entwicklungsmech., Bd. 23, pp. 151-173- McClendon, J. Q. '10 On the Dynamics of Cell Division — II. Changes in Permeability of De- veloping Eggs to Electrolytes. Amer. Journ. Physiol., Vol. 27, pp. 240-275. Ostwald, Wolfgang. '07 Uber das Vorkommen von oxydativen Fermenten in den reifen Geschlechts- zellen von Amphibien und fiber die Rolle dieser Fermente bei den Vorgangen der Entwicklungserregung. Biochem. Zeitsch., Bd. 7, pp. 409-472. Pictet, C. '91 Recherches sur la spermatogenese chez quelques Invertebres de la Mediter- ranee. Mitth. d. zool. Stat. z. Neapel, Bd. 10, pp. 75-152 (see pp. 100-105). Quincke, G. '77 Ueber Diffusion und die Frage, ob Glas fur Case undurchdringlich ist. Poggendorff's Annalen, Bd. 160, pp. 118-123. '02 Ueber unsichtbare Fltissigkeitsschichten und die Oberflachenspannung fliissiger Niederschlage bei Niederschlagsmembranen, Zellen, Colloiden und Gallerten, Ann. d. Physik. 4te Folge, Bd. 7, pp. 631-682, 701-744. Reinke, F. '95 Untersuchung tiber Befruchtung und Furchung des Eies der Echinodermen. Sitzungsber. d. Akad. d. Wiss. z. Berlin, 1895, pp. 625-637. Ries, J. '09 Kinematographie der Befruchtung und Zellteilung. Arch. f. mik. Anat., Bd. 74, pp. 1-31. Robertson, T. B. '12 Studies in the Fertilization of the Eggs of a Sea-Urchin (Slrongylocentrotus purpuratus) by Blood-Sera, Sperm-Extract and Other Fertilizing Agents. Arch. f. Entwicklungsmech., Bd. 35, pp. 64-130. ON SUCCESSIVE DUPLICATE MUTATIONS.1 R. RUGGLES GATES, UNIVERSITY OF LONDON. Nilsson-Ehle2 was the first to formulate the hypothesis of duplicate factors or representatives for the same character. He brought forward evidence from crosses of red and white varieties in certain Swedish strains of wheat, tending to show that in different F2 families plants with red and white grains occurred in the ratios respectively 3:1, 15:1 and 63 :i ; from which he concluded that three independent units for red were present, each of which could produce the color alone. Although his conclusions were criticized by Kajanus,3 yet there remains a strong presumption in their favor, and several other cases of supposed duplicate factors have been described, though these have for the most part rested upon more insecure data than the original instances of Nilsson-Ehle. Certain suggestions have been made concerning the origin of this duplicate or triplicate condition. Emerson and East4 pointed out in general terms that if a factor should become located in a different chromosome or should be affected in any way so as not to be always allelomorphic to itself, then a duplication of determiners would result. Shull5 has listed the supposed cases of duplicate determiners and remarks that such a condition of duplication might also result from "repeated pro- gressive mutations." In the same paper, Shull endeavors to account for the origin simultaneously of a duplicate "gene" 1 Presented before the American Genetic Association, San Francisco meeting, August 3, 1915. 2 Nilsson-Ehle, H., 1909, " Kreuzungsuntersuchungen an Hafer und Weizen," I., Lunds Univ. Arsskrift., N.F., Afd. 2, Bd. 5, Nr. 2, pp. 122. 3 Kajanus, B., 1914, "Zur Kritik des Mendelismus," Zeitschr. f. Abst. u. Vererb., 12: 206-224. 4 Emerson, R. A., and East, E. M., 1913, "The Inheritance of Quantitative Characters in Maize," Agric. Exp. Sta. Nebraska, Research Bull. 2, pp. 120. 6 Shull, Geo. H., 1914, "Duplicate Genes for Capsule-form in Bursa bursa- pastoris," Zeitschr. f. Abst. u. Vererb., 12; 97-149, Figs. 7. 204 ON SUCCESSIVE DUPLICATE MUTATIONS. 2O5 for capsule form in Bursa bur sa- pastor is and, at the same time, of the mutant B. Heegeri. That hypothesis will not, however, apply to the probably more frequent cases in which duplicate factors for a particular character are found without any other mutation having taken place. An explanation will therefore have to be found for the duplicate or triplicate condition in wheat or in any other organisms in which it occurs. It is the purpose of the present paper to discuss more pre- cisely the manner in which such monohybrid characters originate and particularly the way in which they may afterward become duplicate or triplicate. (Enothera rubricalyx affords a typical case of a mutant originating as a monohybrid, probably through a transformation in one chromosome or one pair of chromosomes.1 I have pointed out elsewhere2 that when the duplicate or tripli- cate condition occurs it might be reasonably supposed to have arisen through the same general change having taken place independently in two or three different chromosomes of the x series. In an original mutation of this kind the new character of course forms a pair by contrast with the old unaltered character. If a single chromosome has undergone this change and the new condition is dominant, then a heterozygous mutant Aa will be produced having the new character but splitting in its offspring in a 3:1 ratio. This is the way in which (Enothera rubricalyx originated from (E. rubrinervis, as I have shown elsewhere.1 If now in the mutant race one or both members of a second pair, a' a', of chromosomes undergoes a corresponding change, to A'a', or A' A' then we shall have duplicate factors AA' for the same character, and in the offspring of such individuals the new type and the original type would appear in the ratio of 15:1. A similar mutation in a third pair would give the triplicate condition with a ratio of 63:1. It may be pointed out that this assumption of similar changes in different members of the x series of chromosomes is by no 1 See Gates, R. Ruggles, 1915. "On the Origin and Behaviour of Oenothera rubricalyx," Journ. of Genetics, 4: 353—360. 2 Gates, "The Mutation Factor in Evolution," p. 317, Macmillan, London, IQI5- 206 R. RUGGLES GATES. means an improbable one. It does not assume that the chromo- somes which underwent the change were alike, but merely that they were more nearly alike than the others of the series. That the chromosomes of the x series are actually differentiated there are many lines of evidence to show. One of the most recent is the fact, ascertained by Doncaster and Gray,1 that in certain echinodern crosses particular chromosomes swell up and form vesicles in the strange cytoplasm of the egg or of another species while other chromosomes exhibit no such effect. On the other hand, the materials of the chromosomes obviously possess many similarities which probably outweigh their chemi- cal differences. It may further be pointed out that if both members instead of one member of a pair of chromosomes underwent a change, say in a pollen mother cell, the only difference would be that four instead of two mutated germ cells would result, each of which might function in producing a mutant. It is almost impossible to determine whether one or both members of a pair of chromosomes underwent the change in any particular in- stance, but in either case the original mutant would be hetero- zygous, though continued inbreeding would produce ultimately a homozygous race, as in the case of (E. rubricalyx. This is probably the history of Nilsson-Ehle's wheats which are dup- licate or triplicate for the red color factor in their grains. From the evidence obtained in F2 and F3 in crosses of various Swedish wheats having red kernels, with white-grained varieties, Nilsson-Ehle concludes2 that while the varieties known as Sammet and Grenadier have 3 independent units for red, Extra- Squarehead has only one, since it gave (p. 67) only ratios approxi- mating 3:1. In a later paper3 continuing this work the same author finds (p. 22) that Swedish Binkel wheat contains two factors for red. From one FZ family he grew 94 FS families, with results which may be tabulated as follows: 1 Doncaster, L., and Gray, J., 1913, "Cytological Observations on the Early Stages of Segmentation of Echinus Hybrids," Quart. Journ. Micr. Sci., 58: 483- 510, pis. 28-29. 2 Nilsson-Ehle, H., 1909, Kreuzungsunterschungen an Hafer und Weizen. I. Lunds Univ. Arsskrist., N.F., Afd. 2, Bd. 5, Nr. 2, pp. 122. 3 Nilsson-Ehle, H., 1911, " Kreuzungsuntersuchungen an Hafer und Weizen," II., Lunds Univ. Arsskrift., N.F., Afd. 2, Bd. 7, Nr. 6, pp. 82. OX SUCCESSIVE DUPLICATE MUTATIONS. TABLE I. 207 Expected Ratio of Families. Totals. Expectation. 7 4 4 I 40 families were constant red 23 families split in the ratio 3 : i 25 families split in the ratio 15:1 6 families were constant white 483 : 142 789 : 47 468.75 : 156.25 783-75 : 52.25 It will be seen that the frequency of families is very close to expectation. The totals of the families containing a 15:1 ratio are also very close to expectation, but for the 3:1 families the agreement is not so good. The evidence seems sufficient, how- ever, to justify the conclusion that two factors and two only are here concerned. In the same way evidence is obtained (p. 25) to show that a certain pure line (0406) has in one case a single factor for red and in another case two factors. To use the terminology of Lang, the race is monomerousin one case and dimerous in another. In crosses between the 0406 race and 0234, which was also red,1 ratios of 15:1 and 3:1 were obtained showing that two factors were present, one of which must have been derived from each parent. Hence the 0406 race must in this case have been monomerous. In crosses between 0406 and a white race, 15:1 ratios were again obtained, showing that the 0406 race is now dimerous. The genetic relationships of the strains used in these two crosses is not stated, but a simple explanation is that in the meantime the strain had undergone a second (invisible) mutation. No explanation of the origin of this condition was offered. But there are at least two ways in which the dimerous condition may have been derived from the monomerous: (i) Through a mutation on the part of a second pair of chromosomes, (2) through a re-mating of the chromosome pairs. Later we shall compare the consequence of each of these methods of deriving the duplicate condition. In the first case the duplicate mutation is produced by a change very similar to that which produced the original mutant. In the second case the secondary change is a mechanical one, very different from the primary change which was probably chemical in nature. 1 The results are given in Ber. dent. hot. Gesetts., 29: 65-69, 1911. 2O8 R. RUGGLES GATES. Another pure line of wheat (0290) was also found to be dimer- ous for the red factor in one case and monomerous in another. A race called 0501 was found to be probably trimerous like Swedish Sammet. Nilsson-Ehle considers it scarcely probable that in these two wheats the same three factors for red are present, and thinks that perhaps many more than three independent factors have to be reckoned with. There seems, however, no reason for such an assumption. It appears more probable that corresponding chromosomes undergo the same change in each case so that the factors are all homologous with each other, though of independent origin in the different races. We may, therefore, account for the origin of the duplicate and triplicate "factors" for red in the Swedish wheats by assum- ing that successive mutations have occurred and that in each case the duplicate or triplicate condition has afterward become homozygous and stable through the repeated self-fertilization occurring in later generations. Turning now to the history of (Enothera rubricalyx, it appears that the original monomerous condition has become dimerous in subsequent generations of culture. And it will be seen from the culture records that this has happened independently several times in different lines of descent. It may be worth while first to recapitulate in briefest form the evidence for the original monomeric character of (E. rubricalyx. The original mutant gave an FI offspring of 12 plants, n of which had red buds (R) and one green buds (r). Three of the former selfed produced ¥2 families in which the ratios R :r were respectively 10:5, 14:6, and 33:11. The sum of these three families is 57:22 which is close to a 3 :i ratio and could not reason- ably represent a 15:1 ratio, nor could either of the three ratios individually. Two plants descended from the F* family which yielded 33:11, a perfect 3:1 ratio were used to cross reciprocally with (E. grandiflora, a green budded species of diverse habit. Since these plants were from a family which was obviously mon- omerous, the FI from the cross would either be all R (if the parent was homozygous) or R:r in equal numbers (if the parent was heterozygous). The numbers obtained were 30 R:28r in one cross and 79 R: 71 r in the other. Hence the family which gave ON SUCCESSIVE DUPLICATE MUTATIONS. 2OQ the ratio 33:11 was undoubtedly monomerous and up till that time a single mutation had taken place involving only one pair of chromosomes. It was anticipated that the F2 from rubricalyx X grandiflora and its reciprocal would again yield 3:1 ratios but it was found that in fact there were other ratios as well, the chief of which now appear to be 2:1, 4:1, 5:1 and 15:1. In my discussion of these extensive results1 I was at first inclined to attribute them to an effect of the g andiflora parent in modifying the frequency of inheritance of the R character, and to conclude that since the cross with grandiflora had obviously modified the red-bud char- acter R by dilution in many cases, it must also have modified the frequency with which R would appear. I have since grown a large series of Fg families, the results of which are published in detail elsewhere.2 In the present communication a further analysis of these F2 and F3 ratios will be made, from which it appears that the unexpected ratios obtained in these generations are probably not an effect of the cross with (E. grandiflora, but they result in part from the subsequent occurrence of dupli- cate mutations in rubricalyx. Other ratios, such as 5:1 cannot, however, be fully explained in this way. In this connection it seems desirable to point out that in the inheritance of any character there are two features to be taken into consideration: (i) The nature of the character, and (2) the mechanism of its distribution in the germ cells. Mendelian writers frequently ignore the former, and biometrical writers vitiate their case when they take no account of the latter; but in a complete account of the inheritance of any character both must be considered. As a matter of fact, although crossing with grandiflora probably does not modify the mechanism of trans- mission of R, yet it does seriously and permanently modify the character itself in some cases, as I have shown in previous publications. We may now consider the ratios R:r in the F2 and F$ of (E. rubricalyx X grandiflora and the reciprocal. A further study of 'Gates, R. R., 1914, "Breeding Experiments which Show that Hybridization and Mutation are Independent Phenomena," Zeitschrift /. Abst. u. Vererb., n: 209-279, Figs. 25. 2 "The Mutation Factor in Evolution," pp. 254 ff. 210 R. RUGGLES GATES. these ratios makes it evident that they nearly all fall remarkably close to three or four ratios. So close is the fit that it seems probable that several ratios, such as 5:1, are significant as such, though at present no complete explanation of them can be of- fered. I was formerly inclined to regard some of these ratios as the expression of merely quantitative differences without TABLE II. Fz (Oe. grandi flora X rubricalyx}. Ratios. Expectation. Agreement. Conclusion. 68 : 16 f 63.00 : 21 \ 67.20 : 16.8 3 : i possible 4 : i very near 4 : I 142 : 133 : IS 4 147.00 : 10 128.40 : 8.6 15 : i IS : i 15 = I 15 : I Total 275 : 19 275.60 : 18.4 15 : i perfect Hence 2 families 15 : i i family 4 : i ¥2 (Oe. rubricalyx X grandiflora). (a) 66 : 13 f 59.25 19.75 \6s.84 13.16 3 : I 5 : i perfect 5 : I (b) 45 = 14 44.25 14.75 3 : i nearly perfect 3 : i (c) 47 : 3 Incomplete. In addition 9 dwarfs, i intermedi- ate. (b) 134 : 44 133.50 44-5 3 i perfect 3 : i (a) 67 : 13 f 66.70 13.3 \ 60.00 20 5 i perfect 3 i unlikely 5 : i r 79.20 15.8 5 i very near (a) 82 : 13 J 71.25 23.75 3 i very unlikely 5 : i 1 89.00 6 15 i " r 76.70 15.3 5 i perfect (a) 77 IS ^ 69.00 23 3 I unlikely 5 : i I 86.25 5.75 15 i very unlikely / 45 14 I 134 44 (b)i?9 58 177-75 = 59-25 3 : i wry close 3 : i 66 13 67 13 82 13 77 15 ("288.30 57.7 5 i very close 5 : i (a) 292 54 ^ 259.50 86.5 I 324.40 21.6 3 i unlikely 15 I impossible1 Hence in Fz 2 families 3 : i 4 5:i o constant 1 By "impossible" is meant that the chances against this interpretation, taken in connection with the other results, are so great that for practical purposes it need not be considered. ON SUCCESSIVE DUPLICATE .MUTATIONS. 211 Fa (Oe. grandiflora X rubricalyx). Ratios. Expectation. Agreement. Conclusion. f 229.60 57.4 4 i very near 4 : I 231 : 56 J 239.20 47.8 5 i ? 191.30 95.7 3 i impossible L268.IO 18.9 15 i ("234.40 58.6 4 i very near 4 : i 237 : 56 I 244.20 48.8 5 i ? I 219.75 73-25 3 i improbable ^274.70. 18.3 15 i impossible r 464. oo 116 4 i very near 4 : i Total 468 : 112 •{ 483'3° 9°'7 5 I ""Probable | 435.00 145 3 i improbable ^543-75 36.25 15 i impossible Hence Fs (grandiflora X rubricalyx) 2 families 4 : i Also 4 families constant R 3 constant r 2 intermediate in pigmentation of buds. Fs (Oe. rubricalyx X grandiflora). r 58.70 29-3 2 i very near 2 : i 57 3i •< 66.00 22 3 i ? (.82.50 5-5 IS i impossible 2 3 ? 22 6 21. OO 7 3 i very near 3 : i 112 69 r 120.70 60.3 3 i impossible \I3S-7S 45-25 2 i near? 2:1? 12 2 ? Ci76.oo 35 5 i near 182 29 4 168.80 42.2 4 i impossible 5 : i 1 197. 80 13.2 15 i impossible 4 2 ? 55 2 53-40 3-6 15 i close 15 i 37 2 36.60 2.4 J5 i perfect 15 i 72 6 73-10 4-9 15 i very near 15 i 59 I 56-25 3-75 15 i not very close 15 i Total 223 II 219.40 14.6 15 i very close 15 i r 77-30 38.7 2 i very near 2 I ? 79 37 -s 87.00 29 3 i ? 1 108.75 7-25 15 i impossible 5 3 •) 80 34 (85.50 \ 76.00 28.5 38 3 2 i near i nearer 2:i? Hence in Fs rubricalyx X grandiflora Probably 4 families 2 : i ratio I family 3 : i i 5 = 1" 4 families 15 : I Also 2 families constant for R 8 constant for r i family intermediate 186 plants. 212 R. RUGGLES GATES. more precise significance, but particularly the ratios 5:1 and 15:1 in addition to 3:1 fall so closely into definite categories that the probability of there all being significant is great. The foregoing table contains the F2 and F3 ratios for the various families. Considering these data as a whole, the ratios nearly all fall remarkably close to whole numbers. In the first column of Table II. are given the actual ratios obtained, in the second column the expectation for different ratios, and in the third column the conclusion as to the ratios probably represented in each case. Many of the families are larger than these of Nilsson- Ehle, and in general they appear to fit the various ratios more closely. Considering first the F2 families, it will be seen that those whose ratios are 3:1 or 15:1 are in perfect or almost perfect accord with expectation. This being the case, it seems probable that the 5:1 and 4:1 ratios obtained are significant as such, and in any case they cannot be considered merely wide departures from 3:1. Of the 5:1 ratios the first, second, and fourth are in perfect agreement with 5:1 while the other one is very close, as is also the sum of these four families (292 154) . The significance of these facts is further heightened by the fact that three of these four families (the first, third and fourth) are derived from selfing different flowers of the same FI plant. This is shown in my original record of these experiments,1 and it almost forces the conclusion that in this particular plant as well as others giving similar ratios, R gametes were being produced with greater frequency than r gametes in the ratio 5:3. There is, however, another explanation which will be considered later. In the results of Nilsson-Ehle, on the other hand, the ratios do not fall clearly into such intermediate categories but tend to form a continuous series of ratios as Kajanus pointed out. Thus in one series of crosses2 between black and white glumes involving only monohybrid ratios, the F2 ratios in the 13 families actually range from 2.2:1 (323:144) to 4.1:1 (230:56), yet the total (2468: 795) is fairly close to 3:1. One of these families contained 86 1 Zeitschr.f. Abst. u. Vererb., n, p. 236. 2 " Kreuzungsuntersuchungen," I, p. 18. OX SUCCESSIVE DUPLICATE MUTATIONS. 213 black (B): 22 white (B). F3 offspring were grown from each of these 1 08 plants and the results showed their composition to have been as follows: 36 BB:5oBb:22 bb. From this result Nilsson-Ehle concludes that there was a preponderance of "black" gametes over "white" ones. But a series of F3 families in another cross gave the reverse condition, 26 BB:6oBb: 33 bb, from wrhich the conclusion is drawn that wrhite gametes were here more numerous than black ones. Even though these re- sults offset each other yet they cannot be referred merely to chance fluctuations in ratios. But no further explanation of them was offered. It will be shown later that in my crosses of (E. rubricalyx and (E. grandiflora these deviating ratios do not offset each other but are all consistent with the hypothesis that R gametes are being produced with greater frequency than r gametes. Returning now to Table II. the first ratio (68:16) is not a very bad fit for 3:1, although exceedingly close to 4:1. It might easily pass for 3:1 without further comment were it not for the fact that two other ratios in this table are in very close agree- ment with 4:1 while they depart very widely from 3:1. The sum of these two ratios (468:112) is very close to 4:1 while it is highly improbable as a 3:1 ratio, the more so since the actual 3:1 ratios are almost in precise agreement with expectation. Among the three ¥% families from (E. grandiflora X rubricalyx, two show a 15:1 ratio and one a 4:1 or perhaps a 3:1 ratio. Hence it might be supposed that the rubricalyx plant which was used as pollen parent, already possessed duplicate factors for red. But this was not the case. That plant was in fact heterozygous for a single factor, since when crossed with grandiflora it gave an FI of 79R : yir, which is as near to equality as could be ex- pected. The full history of the rubricalyx individuals used for this and the reciprocal cross has been given in pedigree form in another paper,1 to which reference should be made. It may be said that in both cases they are descended from the family which contained 33R : nr. One member of this family was pollinated by nanella and produced a family of 42 plants. One of the latter (No. IV., 2), which was a perfect rubricalyx in appearance but 1 Gates, Zeitschr. f. Abst. u. Vererb., u : opp. p. 216 and on p. 217. 214 R- RUGGLES GATES. carried dwarfing latent, was used to pollinate (E. grandiflora. Since the F% offspring of this cross gave 15:1 ratios in two families, while the FI was a 1:1 ratio, duplicate mutations must have intervened between these two generations. The two plants which were the parents of the families containing 142 R: 15 r and 1 13 R: 4 r respectively must have possessed the duplicate factor in all their germ cells, so that they were heterozygous for R and R'. Their composition might then be written RrRV. As pointed out earlier in this paper, such a condition might have arisen (a) through the transformation of a chromosome belonging to a second pair, (b) through an exchange of mates on the part of two pairs of chromosomes. We may now examine the comparative credibility of these two alternatives. There are certain difficulties with either hypothesis, one of which is that the transformation from the monomerous to the dimerous condition, whether effected by chemical or mechanical means, must apparently have taken place early in the ontogeny, before definitive germ cells are formed. The alternative hypothesis would be that all the germ cells had undergone the transformation simultaneously and independently, which one cannot believe possible. There is, however, one consideration which makes it appear probable that the duplicate condition for R is not usually arrived at through a transformation of a new chromosome, but rather through a redistribution of the chromosomes. The 15:1 ratio can only be obtained from an RrRV parent, in which both duplicate factors are heterozygous. It would therefore be necessary to assume when a 15:1 family is derived from a 3:1 family, that a chromosome belonging to a new pair had under- gone a chemical transformation while its mate and the mate of the original modified chromosome were unaffected, i. e., that the condition RrrV became altered directly to RrRV. This is very unlikely. On the other hand, as I showed long ago,1 the chromosomes in (Enothera are very loosely paired during the reduction division, and moreover irregular chromosome distri- butions have been shown to occur at this time (as in the pro- duction of (E. mut. /a/a). I also (/. c.) pointed out the probability 1 "A Study of Reduction in QLnothera rubrinervis," Bot. Gazette, 46: 1-34, pis. 3, 1908. ON SUCCESSIVE DUPLICATE MUTATIONS. 215 that exchanges of chromosomes of different pairs but without change in number would take place under these conditions. By such a mismating or exchange of mates on the part of two chromosome pairs, a plant which was homozygous (RR) for one factor would give rise to plants which were heterozygous for duplicate factors (RrRV). This is then what has probably occurred in the cases where plants from a 3:1 family have given rise to 15:1 ratios. The frequency with which such mismating occurs in CEnothera may thus be estimated. It is known that the chromosomes of CEnothera are in pairs (doubtless of paternal and maternal origin) throughout the somatic divisions, and the paired arrangement is probably a feature of the first mitosis after fertilization. If, then, a plant which would have been homozygous for a single factor (RR) becomes transformed into one which is heterozygous for dupli- cate factors (RrRV) and so gives a ratio 15:1 in its offspring, the most likely assumption is that at the time of fertilization the two R chromosomes, instead of becoming paired with each other, each paired with another (r) chromosome. Hence in this case the regrouping of chromosomes probably occurred not during meiosis where it would have to occur simultaneously in all the germ cells, but as a feature of fertilization or the first mitosis of the embryo. It will be shown later, however, that mismatings of the chromosome pairs probably also occur during meiosis and so modify the 3:1 ratio. To sum up, it appears that when a 15:1 family is derived directly from a plant in a 3:1 family, the remating of the chromosomes must have occurred at fertilization or soon afterwards; but when, for example, a 4:1 or a 5:1 family is derived from a 3:1 family, this may be accounted for by a cer- tain amount of remating of chromosomes during meiosis. The method above described will also apply to the origin of duplicate and triplicate factors in wheat and is perhaps more probable than the successive chemical transformation of dif- ferent chromosomes. There is, however, a method of testing between these two possibilities. If the duplicate condition arises through a regrouping of the chromosome pairs, then, as has been mentioned, a race or a plant homozygous (RR) for one factor will give rise to a plant heterozygous for two factors 216 R. RUGGLES GATES. (RrRV). On the other hand, if the chemical transformation of a fresh chromosome takes place in a homozygous monomeric plant (RR), then the dimerous individual derived from such a monomerous plant should have the constitution RRRV.1 It would be possible to determine between these two alternatives by breeding tests. If the constitution of the plant is RrRV its offspring should give a 15:1 ratio. If it is RRR'r' they would all be red in FI and F2. But plants having the former formula could also be produced by the mismating of chromosome-pairs during meiosis in RR plants. If we now return to the table (p. 210) and examine the F2 from the reciprocal cross (rubricalyx X grandiflora) we find a total absence of 15:1 ratios, showing that not only was the rubricalyx parent of this cross monomerous but its offspring remained so. The parent of this cross was a member (No. IV., 8) of the monom- erous family 33:11. As will be seen from the table, two of the F2 families from rubricalyx X grandiflora gave perfect or almost perfect 3:1 ratios. Four others gave 5:1 ratios, three of which were perfect and the other very close to expectation as already pointed out. I have at present no further explanation of these 5:1 ratios to offer, but it seems probable that their significance will later become apparent. Ratios more or less in excess of 3:1 could be obtained from plants homozygous for one factor, if there was a tendency for mismating of the chromosomes in meiosis. But this will not account for the definiteness of the 5:1 ratios obtained. Turning to the F3 of grandiflora X rubricalyx the full data are given in my book (p. 255). Four families were constant for R, 3 constant for r, 2 families numbering respectively 283 and 20 plants bred true to an intermediate condition, and 2 families split in the ratio 4:1, as shown in the table (p. 21 1). The excess of R's in the last two families is a significant excess over 3:1, however it is brought about. In the F3 of rubricalyx X grandiflora, four families give ratios nearest 2:1, one family near 3:1, one near 5:1 and four very close to 15:1. Whatever the significance of the 2:1 and 5:1 ratios in 1 We have already found it highly improbable that a plant Rr could be directly transformed chemically into RrR'r', since we should anticipate that the chromo- some r would undergo a mutation before the chromosome r'. ON SUCCESSIVE DUPLICATE MUTATIONS. 2IJ these families, the appearance of 15:1 ratios in the Fs of this cross is of much interest, since the F% contained no families which could reasonably be construed as containing duplicate factors, except the one having the incomplete ratio 47:3. Reference to the pedigree numbers1 shows that the first two are derived from the FZ family No. 60 in which the ratio is doubtful, the third is derived from selfing a plant in the Fg family No. 62, and the fourth from selfing one in family No. 63. In these two families the ratios were respectively 67:13 and 82:13, both of which are shown (p. 210) to be very near 5 :i ratios. The appear- ance of these 15:1 ratios in F^ from 5:1 families can be explained if we assume that independent duplicate mutations have occurred in the F% families 60, 62, and 63. This must happen as pre- viously outlined, through a plant which is homozygous for one factor giving rise to a plant which is heterozygous for two; or in other words, through the rearrangement of a pair of homologous chromosomes so that they belong to different pairs. Another point which will be explained by the present hypoth- esis is the difference in the depth of color in homozygous red- budded races. Thus in the FS families 93 and 95, - containing respectively 280 and 312 plants, the latter were constantly darker red than the former. The latter family was doubtless homozygous for duplicate factors (RRR'R/), or at least RRRV, since the family from which it was derived yielded 15:1 ratios. The former family was on the other hand probably homozygous for a single factor (RR) and hence not so densely red-pigmented. It will thus be seen that in several instances 15:1 families have been obtained from the offspring of 3:1 or 5:1 families. All such cases can be explained by assuming that a duplicate mutation has intervened. The original mutation by which deep red buds in (Enothera first appeared is an extremely rare occur- rence, having occurred but once in all cultures of (Enothera. When, however, a chromosome has once undergone this change it is reasonable to suppose that other chromosomes in the same nucleus could without difficulty take on an analogous trans- formation. The whole mechanism is, however, at hand in the 1 See "The Mutation Factor in Evolution," p. 256. "-L. c., p. 255. 2l8 R. RUGGLES GATES. meiotic divisions, for transforming the original 3:1 ratio into a 15:1 by merely redistributing the chromosome pairs. In concluding this paper it is desirable to compare the related but different results recently obtained by Honing,1 with two varieties of Canna indica which are naturalized in Sumatra. One variety has green leaves while in the other the leaves have a broad red margin. From the offspring of plants of the latter variety Honing obtained ratios red : green of 3 :i , 9 7 and 27 137. The same ratios were obtained in crossing the two varieties. These ratios are accounted for by the hypothesis that the cooperation of three "factors" is necessary to produce the red margin. If these are located in chromosomes belonging to three different pairs, then the resulting ratio should be 27 red: 37 green, since the character can only appear in the presence of all three factors A, B, C. On the other hand, if all three factors are located in the same chromosome a 3:1 ratio would be obtained, while if two of them were in one chromosome and the third in a chromo- some of a second pair, the ratio would be 9:7. It was found that in certain cases plants in families having a 3:1 ratio gave rise in the next generation to a 9:7 or 27:37 family. In such cases one may assume that a mutation has taken place resulting in a redistribution of the determiners, the three which were present in one chromosome being rearranged so that they are in chromosomes belonging to two or three different pairs. So far as I am aware, this is the first experimental evidence that an actual rearrangement of the chromomeres in the chromo- somes is one of the kinds of change which the nucleus may undergo, the case being somewhat different from Morgan's well-known phenomena of "crossing over" in Drosophila. Further experiments are necessary to test the nature of this evidence for the occurrence of mutations in which such a re- arrangement of the nuclear material can take place. SUMMARY. Nilsson-Ehle was the first to show that duplicate and triplicate factors for red are present in certain strains of wheat. He 1 Honing, J. A., 1915, " Kreuzungsversuche mit Canna- Varietaten," Rec. Trav. bot. Neerlandais, Vol. 12: Livr. i, pp. 26. ON SUCCESSIVE DUPLICATE MUTATIONS. 219 found, moreover, that the same strain may be in one case monom- erous and in another case dimerous for this character; and that while, for example, Grenadier wheat possessed three independent units for red, Extra-Squarehead possessed only one. The origin of the original ' ' factor ' 'for red may be accounted for in the wheats as in (Enothera rubricalyx, through the chemical transformation of one chromosome or a pair of homologous chromosomes. The duplicate condition for the character R may have arisen (i) through a chemical mutation in a second pair of chromosomes, (2) through a re-mating of the chromosomes (RR) forming a homo- zygous pair. The latter method is for various reasons the more probable. Although the original (Enothera rubricalyx was a monohybrid and continued so for at least two generations, yet in subsequent generations involved in crosses with (E. grandiflora, 15:1 or di-hybrid ratios were derived from the offspring of members of 3:1 families. This can best be accounted for by supposing that in a plant (RR) homozygous for one factor, a re-grouping of the chromosome pairs occurred. This re-grouping involves merely an exchange of mates on the part of the chromosomes RR so that they now belong to different pairs. The formula for the plant may now be written RrRV, i. e., the plant is heterozygous for two independent units for red and its offspring will give a 15:1 ratio. The second mutation, producing the duplicate condition for R, is thus probably a purely mechanical process, while the origi- nal mutation which produced the "factor' R is a chemical change of wholly different nature. It is possible that in some cases the duplicate and triplicate conditions also arise through the chemical transformation of additional chromosomes. When a 15:1 family arises from a 3:1 or 5:1 family, as has happened several times in (E. rubricalyx hybrids, it is necessary to assume that the regrouping or remating of chromosome pairs which led from the monohybrid to the dihybrid condition, took place at fertilization, or at any rate early in the ontogeny, and is then handed down to the germ cells by mitosis. The chro- mosomes are known to be paired in the somatic divisions, and it seems probable that the manner of pairing set up in fer- 220 R. RUGGLES GATES. tilization continues in this case throughout the ontogeny, though this is not true for all organisms. Otherwise it would be necessary to assume that when a plant in a 3:1 family gives rise to a 15:1 family all its germ cells have simultaneously undergone a mis- mating of the chromosome pairs during meiosis, a highly im- probable event. In the F2 and F3 hybrids of CE. rubricalyx and CE. grandiflora, in addition to 3:1 and 15:1 ratios, 2:1, 4:1 and 5:1 ratios occur. The 5:1 ratios at least seem to be significant, indicating that R and r gametes are regularly being produced in the ratio 5:3, or that a certain amount of re-grouping of the R chromosomes is regularly occurring during meiosis. Vol. XXIX. October, 1915. No. 4 BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN REACTIONS AND RESISTANCE OF FISHES IN THEIR NATURAL ENVIRONMENT TO ACIDITY, ALKALINITY AND NEUTRALITY. MORRIS M. WELLS. PAGE. I. Introduction 221 II. Apparatus and Methods 222 A. Reaction Experiments 222 B. Resistance Experiments 225 III. The Water 225 IV. Presentation of Data 231 A . Reaction Experiments 231 1. Reaction to Acids 231 (a) To Carbonic Acid 231 (b) To Sulfuric Acid 232 (c) To Acidity in Distilled Water 234 2. Reaction to Alkalies 235 (a) In Neutral Water 235 (b) In Strongly Acid Water 235 3. Conclusions Drawn from the Reaction Experiments 239 B. Resistance Experiments 239 1. Resistance to Acids 239 2. Resistance to Alkalies 240 3. Resistance to Neutrality 240 V. General Discussion 243 VI. General Conclusions 254 VII. Acknowledgments and Bibliography 254 I. INTRODUCTION. The present paper is the first of a series that is to deal with the relation of fishes to ions in the natural environments. It is purposed to point out some of the close correlations which exist between the physiology of fishes and their behavior, and to present evidence concerning the importance of such corre- lations in biological investigation in general. The data pre- sented in the following pages deal with the reactions and re- sistance of fresh water fishes to acidity, neutrality and alkalinity; 221 222 MORRIS M. WELLS. the discussion of the data shows that the phenomena outlined receive much support from the work of other investigators and that the environmental factors which are important to fresh water fishes are probably of importance to many, if not all, other organisms as well. The investigation has been carried on at the University of Illinois, in Professor V. E. Shelford's laboratory. The work has been done in connection with another line of inquiry regarding the reactions and resistance of fishes to salts. The results of this second investigation will appear as the second paper of the series. II. APPARATUS AND METHODS. Two general types of experiments have been run, namely, reaction experiments, and resistance experiments. A. REACTION EXPERIMENTS. This method of experimentation was devised by Shelford and Allee ('13) and may be designated as the "gradient method." In brief the procedure is as follows: A solution gradient is es- tablished in an observation tank, the fish introduced, and its movements graphed. The graph, together with notes taken at the time, makes up the experimental record. Fig. i shows the type of tank used. A similar tank was used by Shelford and Powers ('15) in their experiments with marine fishes. A black hood screens the tank, the movements of the fishes being viewed through slits in the front of the hood. The tank has a plate glass front and is lighted by symmetrical lights placed above. A plate-glass cover fits into the top and rests against the surface of the water. This cover is useful in experiments with gaseous gradients as it lessens the vertical gradient due to escape of gas at the surface. The water flows into the tank through the openings (inlets) in the ends, then toward the middle; at the middle the water from the two ends mixes, the water from each end drifting somewhat past the middle, thus forming the gradient. The water flows out through the exits (outlets) in the bottom and at the top of the tank. An experiment consists of first establishing the grad- ient, and then introducing the fish and graphing its movements. REACTION AND RESISTANCE OF FISHES. 223 In establishing the gradient the flow at each end of the tank was fixed at 500 c.c. per minute in practically all the experiments. The flow of tap water was regulated to 500 c.c. per min. at one *j -/ )-i 01 4J IU M O •4-t o 0> rn — ' ^_J o J2 CJ 0 <=^i i — ^= ET^ s •o to 4_i T3 "w ° •o \ « OJ " *G g > o 0 '' '' ! !|i| i|i II ,"l II '' I ' , 1 ' 0, 1 1 ' fl 1 ' n' ' n |,i 0 ' ' 1 ''1 Ij 'V1 ilsk Sty «— ^ \x /p — -> /« SC Sc OulUls sc rte>v in which the reaction experiments were performed. ~ ~ a SJ 4J o 4-> j.-. _o "w 1 n •o g to to cS 5 O -4-> >» ja •o V — 0 o -t-» OJ — • 14-I o o cd J3 M 3 O — -u O •o a -w OJ 4J O -4_> S 0 •r ri 2 liberated in this gradient made the water so acid that the fishes were soon overcome, and died if not removed from the tank. However at first they gave a decidedly negative reaction to the acid end. (2) H2SOt to Liberate 40 c.c. COZ per Liter vs. Neutral Water.— The fishes reacted much as they did in gradients of aerated vs. unaerated water. They were very negative to the acid end. (3) H%SOi to Liberate 4 c.c. C02 per Liter vs. Neutral Water (Graphs 4 and 5, Chart /.). — Eighteen experiments of this sort were run. Of the 18 graphs, 14 show that the fishes spent 90 per cent, of the time in the acid half of the tank; 3 show more than 50 per cent, of the time in this half and I (small blue-gill) shows an 80 per cent, preference for the neutral end. That the fishes are negative to neutral water is thus confirmed. To ascertain the chemical reaction of the water at the point in the tank where the fishes turned back from the neutral end, numerous samples were titrated from this region, during the experiment. They showed that the water at this point contained about I c.c. of CC>2 per liter. The graphs shown in Chart 1. are typical for all. 234 MORRIS M. WELLS. Bluegill. Bull-head. Crappie. Bluegill. Bluegill and Bull-heads. Bull-head. be" I", ^ t/2 TtU . be " 55 « Control. CHART I. Showing the reactions of the fishes to different degrees of acidity. The gradient is between the two kinds of water, indicated at the top of each graph. Numbers at the left of the chart indicate time in minutes. Strongly acid = 18 c.c. CCh per liter; moderately acid = 8-10 c.c. per liter; slightly acid = 2-3 c.c. per liter; and neutral water = actual neutrality to i c.c. per liter. Dotted lines indicate that fish was driven. (c) Reaction to Acidity in Distilled Water. — The distilled water, which was available in quantity from the chemistry department, was not rapidly toxic to the fishes and since the foregoing results are of some general biological importance, it was decided to repeat the experiments in distilled water. This water was faintly acid with CO2 containing 2-3 c.c. per liter. It contained no salts ; so the addition of a strong acid resulted in no complications such as those discussed in the case of the tap water. A number of experiments was performed with various strengths of acid and alkali. The neutral portion of the tank was kept track of by means of titra- tions and the reactions of the fishes to this neutral region es- REACTION AND RESISTANCE OF FISHES. 235 pecially noted. The results are presented in Table 1 1.; in brief they are as follows. The fishes spent practically all the time in the acid portion of the tank, turning back from the alkaline end at a point just on the acid side of neutrality. They did not, how- ever, select the highest acidity available, but swam back and forth in the tank betAveen neutrality on the one hand and about .OOO2N HoSO4 on the other. The small amount of COa present in the distilled water may be neglected in the presence of the much more ionized acid. At the range of dilution used in these experiments, carbonic acid would have to be about 1,400 times as concentrated as sulfuric acid, to give an equal concentration of H ion. TABLE II. SHOWING THE REACTIONS OF FISHES TO ACIDITY AND ALKALINITY IN DISTILLED WATER. Acid Used. Concentration. Reaction. .00004 N Negative: choose higher acidity. .0001 N Positive: some fishes choose this concentra- tration in preference to either higher or lower acidity. H2SO4 .0005 N Very negative. H2SO4 .0002 N Still very negative. H2SO4 .00005 N Positive when neutral water is the other choice. The fishes used did not select alkaline water in any case except when the only other choice was neutrality. Then they spent most of the time on the alkaline side, rather than at the neutral point. 2. Reactions to Alkalies. (a) Alkalies in Neutral Water, (i) Na2COs (.01 N) in Neutral Water vs. Neutral Water. — Six experiments were run with this combination. The results were rather indefinite. However, the graphs as a whole show a slight preference for the alkaline half of the tank. As has been pointed out already, the fishes are nega- tive to the neutral water, and these experiments confirm this reaction, even though the only other choice is alkalinity. (b~) Alkalies in Strongly Acid Water. — In this water which is acid with CO2 (18 c.c. per liter), the first action of the alkali will be to neutralize the acid. Thus a small amount of alkali introduced at one end will simply produce an acid gradient by 236 MORRIS M. WELLS. lessening the acidity at this end. Eighteen c.c. of CO2 equals an .0008 N solution. In most cases, the concentrations of alkali used have been much greater than this and the amount used up in neutralizing the acid may be looked upon as negligible. In some experiments, to be cited, the acid factor is of much impor- tance. (1) NazCOz (.01 N) in Strongly Acid Water vs. Strongly Acid Water (Graph I, Chart II.}. — The fishes stayed in the middle of the tank, coming to the surface very little. The gradient was acid at one end and alkaline at the other. Titrations showed that the fishes spent most of the time on the acid side of neutrality. (2) NaiCOz (.002 N} in Strongly Acid Water vs. Strongly Acid Water (Graph 2, Chart II.}. Fifteen experiments were run with this combination. The graphs show that the fishes spent most of the time nearer the alkaline end than before, but titra- tion showed that they were merely following the neutral point, remaining on the acid side most of the time. (3) Na^COz (.0005 N) in Strongly Acid Water vs. Strongly Acid Water (Graph 3, Chart II.). This concentration of alkali was just a little more than enough to neutralize the acid in the water of the alkaline end. The end was really slightly acid, however, from the diffusion of more acid from the acid end of the gradient. The fishes moved into the so-called alkaline (really slightly acid) end and remained there during the experi- ment. This was true for all the fishes used. (4) NaHC03 (.oiN) in Strongly Acid Water vs. Strongly Acid Water. — This salt is neutral to phenolphthalein as has been pointed out in the preceding discussion. A number of experi- ments, recorded both by graphs and readings at short intervals, were run with it. The results were not at all definite. The fishes seemed to be indifferent to this bicarbonate in acid water, or else they were not at all stimulated by its presence. (5) NHiOH in Moderately Acid Water (Made it Faintly Alka- line} vs. Moderately Acid Water (Graphs 4 and 5, Chart II.}. Ten experiments with this alkali were run, to check up Shelford and Alice's work ('13) with it. They say (p. 252) that the fishes (Abramis) did not react to ammonia in a concentration which •caused them to turn on their sides after an hour or more. In REACTION AND RESISTANCE OF FISHES. 237 my experiments, I found also that the fishes do not react to this alkali with the precision found for the other alkalies used. liull-head. Sun-fish. Crappie. Bluegill. Bluegill. liluegill. H || |8 g'D W U g> Q M-c 5 • •r" rt *^ 5. - WS z ' S >-T3 "C ^ •- Control. CHART II. Showing the reactions of the fishes to alkalies. The gradient was between the two kinds of water indicated at the top of each graph. Strongly acid water = 18 c.c. CO2 per liter; and moderately acid = 8-10 c.c. per liter. Numbers to left = time in minutes. In the first experiments a .005 N solution was run into one end of the tank. The fishes selected the middle of the tank for the most part, though one blue-gill was positive to the ammonia end. The concentration of OH ion was of course very low, with so small a concentration of so weakly ionized a base, and since other experiments have shown that blue-gills are less negative to neutrality than are other fishes, this reaction is not surprising. The ammonia concentration was raised to .01 N and the fishes, blue-gill included, moved toward the tap-water end of the tank. Later the concentration of the alkali was 238 MORRIS M. WELLS. raised to .02 N, but even now the avoidance of the alkali end was not nearly so definite as in the experiments with the other alkalies. Graphs 4 and 5 (Chart II.) show this indefinite reaction very clearly. In the .02 TV gradient, the fishes were soon overcome by the toxicity of the water, which they selected, and they died there if not removed. The fact that fishes fail to recognize ammonia in solution is of considerable importance, for this substance is being introduced into fish waters in many kinds of sewage. Furthermore it will be shown in the second paper of this series, that the gas has not lost its toxicity even when it has been converted into its various salts. The chemical explanation of the failure of the fishes to recognize and react to the presence of fatal concen- trations of the hydrate in solution is probably due to the fact that the concentration of ammonia as gas, reaches a fatal con- centration before the concentration of OH ion stimulates the fishes sufficiently to cause them to react negatively. They do not appear to react to the gas itself. Noyes ('13, pp. 203-4) states that ammonia dissolves in water, in part, without chemical change and that it is probable that a large part of the ammonia exists, as such, in the solution. He quotes Moore ('07) as cal- culating that only 30-40 per cent, of the ammonia exists as am- monium hydroxide, NH4OH, at 20° C. Noyes thinks that the per cent, may be even less than this. Again, the solution of ammonia diffuses through the water with great rapidity; much more rapidly than do most other substances. To determine the rate of diffusion, a little phe- nolphthalein was added to the aspirator bottle (AB, Fig. i) containing the ammonia solution. The pink solution could be seen as it moved through the tank, and in less than a minute it had spread over the entire surface, and to a lesser extent, had penetrated the deeper water. Because of this rapid diffusion, no perfect gradient could be established with this substance. It may also be noted that ammonia behaves just opposite from the salts, the latter spreading along the bottom. In the am- monia experiments, the fishes seldom approach the surface, while in strong carbon dioxide gradients, they spend much time gulping the surface film. Shelford and Alice ('13, p. 231) state REACTION AND RESISTANCE OF FISHES. 239 that in open tanks the amount of CQz at the surface is markedly less than at deeper levels. 3. Conclusions Drawn from the Reaction Experiments. The reaction experiments recorded in the previous pages sug- gest the following conclusions. (i) Fresh-water fishes are negative to neutrality in favor of either slight acidity or slight alkalinity. Their normal choice is slight acidity' (about .00005 N H2SO4 or .0001 N CO2). (2) Species of fishes differ in the de- gree of acidity selected. Blue-gills select water that is but very slightly acid (1-2 c.c. CC>2 per liter, i. e., .0001 N carbonic acid) while crappies select a concentration of from 4-6 c.c. CO? per liter. (3) The principal stimuli to which fishes react are H and OH ions. They do not react to ammonia, as a gas in solution. B. RESISTANCE EXPERIMENTS. It has been pointed out that the stock of fishes did not live well in the aquaria when these were supplied with water, which was neutral, or nearly so; to determine more exactly the reasons for the high mortality, between 50 and 60 experiments were performed. Some of these experiments lasted through a number of weeks, while others were finished in a few hours. The fishes were placed in different concentrations of acid and alkali in partly aerated wrater (from the aquaria) and in distilled water. i. Resistance to Acids. The resistance of fishes to carbonic acid has been worked out (Wells, '13) and it was decided to try the effects of other acids. Ten experiments with sulfuric acid in distilled water are sum- marized in Table III. The table shows that there is a concentra- TABLE III. SHOWING THE RESISTANCE OF FISHES (3 GRAM BLUE-GILLS) TO HiSO4 IN DISTILLED WATER. Concentration of Acid. Dying Time in Hours. .001 N 3.5 .0005 N 7.0 .0002 N 42.0 .00015 N 60.0 .000075 ^ Alive and vigorous at end of a month. 240 MORRIS M. WELLS. tion of this acid in distilled water, at which the fishes in question live as well as though in tap water. Higher concentrations of acid are fatal, the time required to kill the fishes being propor- tional to the hydrogen ion concentration. 2. Resistance to Alkalies. In a .001 N solution of KOH in distilled water, a 3 gram blue- gill lived 4 hrs. and 25 min. In a .0005 N solution, a fish of the same size was alive at the end of 10 days. Titration at this time showed that the water had become acid to phenolphthalein from the CO2 given off in the metabolism of the fish. The experi- ment was discontinued. To make sure that the fish in the first experiment had not been killed by the toxic potassium ion, another 3 gram blue-gill was placed in a .01 N solution of NaHCOs in distilled water. At the beginning, this solution was neutral, but it was expected that the bicarbonate would dissociate and the solution would become slightly alkaline from the carbonate thus formed. A blank control, containing the same amount of bicarbonate, but no fish, was run. The fish in the experiment died on the third day. Titration showed that the water had become .0009 N alkaline. The control was .001 N alkaline. Blue-gills therefore do not live well in water which is even very slightly alkaline. 3. Resistance to Neutrality. The foregoing experiments, together with many facts recorded in the literature, suggested the possibility that the fact that it is neutral may have something to do with the toxicity of distilled water. Thirteen experiments were performed to test this pos- sibility in a preliminary way. The facilities available did not make it possible to experiment with absolutely neutral water, but the results obtained are suggestive, as neutrality was ap- proached very closely in some cases. Most of the experiments were performed with water that came from a copper still and will be referred to as once-distilled water. A few experiments were performed with a much purer water which was the once- distilled water redistilled in a better still and coming in contact with little copper. In neither kind of water could the amount REACTION AND RESISTANCE OF FISHES. 24! of copper have been especially large, however, for small blue-gills lived in both kinds as well as in tap-water, so long as the water was slightly acid. A comparison of the conductivities of the two kinds of water showed that the once-distilled had a conductivity1 of 600 X io~r while the conductivity of the twice-distilled was only 10 X io~7. These conductivities are for 25° C. The conductivity of the water probably does not indicate the amount of copper present however for the metal is in all likelihood present in the col- loidal state. Mengarini and Scala ('12) have shown that a num- ber of metals, including copper, form a colloidal solution with distilled water even at room temperature, and especially in the absence of air. The conditions in a still would be especially favorable for the reaction, since the temperature is high and air excluded. The addition of an acid to a colloidal solution would tend to precipitate the colloid, and this undoubtedly explains in part the effect of addition of acid in making distilled water less toxic,2 as it will be shown that it does. Since, however, it has been shown (Bullot, '04) that distilled water which contains no copper is still toxic to organisms, other factors must be con- cerned. The evidence of the experiments presented in the present paper, indicates that the neutrality of the water is one of these factors. It has been suggested in the preceding pages that the blue- gills and crappies differ in respect to the hydrogen ion concen- tration which they select and their resistance to the distilled water bears out this point as the crappies die in it in a day or so, while the blue-gills live indefinitely. (i) Experiments with Once-distilled Water. — This water was slightly acid to phenolphthalein and was neutral to methyl orange. Its toxicity was tested by placing fishes in jars containing a liter of the water. A 1 2-gram crappie died in this 1 The conductivity of pure water is I X io~7. 2 Locke ('95) calls attention to the fact that poisonous distilled water may lose its poisonous properties (if due to copper) by long boiling, and especially when brought into contact with sulphur, carbon, manganic oxide, cotton wool, silk, and other substances. The effect is very probably again due to the precipitation of the colloidal copper. 242 MORRIS M. WELLS. water in 2 days, but when this same liter of water was divided into two parts and a 3-gm. blue-gill placed in each part, both fishes were normal at the end of a month. In Table IV. is given a summary of a number of experiments performed with blue-gills in once-distilled water. TABLE IV. SHOWING THE RESISTANCE OF SMALL BLUE-GILLS (3-5 GRAMS) TO DISTILLED WATER THAT is BARELY ACID WITH COz. Conditions of Expt. Fish Placed in, Resistance of Fishes. 1. Freshly distilled water Normal after 5 days; expt. discont. 2. Boiled distilled water Normal after 5 days; expt. discont. 3. Distilled water plus Na2COs to make neutral Normal after 2 days. Water acid again. Added a little NaHCO3 to (3) to keep neutral Dead on loth day. 4. In dist. water as in (i) Normal after 30 days. Table IV. shows that the once-distilled water is not greatly if at all toxic to the blue-gills, but experiment 3 shows that these fishes cannot live in the water if it is slightly alkaline. This same distilled water is rapidly toxic to the crappies and sun- fishes, however, as was shown in an experiment already described and in those which follow. This lack of resistance of the sun- fishes in particular is a complete reversal of the ordinary specific resistance of these fishes as compared with the blue-gills, for in carbon monoxide, ethylene, sulphur dioxide, etc., the sun- fishes are much more resistant than are the blue-gills. On January 30, a liter of water (once-distilled) was made .00005 N acid with H2SO4 and another liter left as it came from the still. An 8-gram crappie was placed in each jar. The fish in the pure distilled water was dead in 12 hrs. while the one in the distilled water made acid, lived for 65 hrs. Several other •experiments of this sort gave similar results, showing that the crappies cannot live in the neutral distilled water when it is pure, as well as they can when it is made slightly acid. It is very probable that sightly higher concentrations of acid than those used would have prolonged the lives of these fishes even more successfully than the .00005 N DUt as the stock of fishes was running low, these experiments were reserved for another time. REACTION AND RESISTANCE OF FISHES. 243 An experiment with small bullheads is very interesting. Normally the bullheads are perhaps the most resistant fresh- water fishes known. In the reaction experiments they selected a rather low concentration of hydrogen ion but were decidedly on the acid side of neutrality. In the pure distilled water a bullhead (4 in. long) lived 8 days; another in distilled water made .00005 N acid, lived for 20 days. (2) Resistance to Doubly Distilled Water. — This water was less toxic to the crappies than was the once distilled water, as it contained less colloidal copper. It has been pointed out that the toxicity of the once-distilled water was lessened by the addition of acid, partly because the acid precipitated the col- loidal copper. The experiments indicate further, however, that the neutrality of the water must be reckoned with also. This is again brought out, and more definitely, by a few experiments with the twice-distilled water. A quantity of this water was placed in a large bottle and a solution of barium hydroxid was suspended over it. At the end of a week the water was practically neutral. Two portions were taken in 500 c.c. Erlenmeyer flasks and a small bullhead (2.5 in.) placed in each. One portion was left neutral and the other made slightly acid with HoSO4. The fish in the neutral water lived 16 days and the one in the acid water 19 days. A few other experiments were performed with the twice-distilled water and all gave similar results. The stock of fishes was about exhausted, however, and further ex- periments were delayed until another time. V. GENERAL DISCUSSION. The fact that in natural bodies of water the chemical reaction of the water may vary from alkalinity through neutrality to acidity or the reverse, makes the practical importance of a knowl- edge of the reactions and resistance of fishes and other organisms to such chemical conditions an obvious one. From the experi- ments and discussion which have gone before, it is clear that water which gives an alkaline reaction to phenolphthalein for any length of time during the year, is undesirable as a home for most fresh-water fishes. On the other hand, marine fishes (Shelford and Powers, '15) with the exception of the anadro- 244 MORRIS M. WELLS. mous species, probably would not survive in water which was even faintly acid. Since algae and other phytoplankton forms (Birge and Juday, 'n and '12) may cause a body of water to be- come partially or wholly alkaline, through their ability to dis- sociate the bicarbonates, vegetation in fish waters assumes a line of importance heretofore little considered. The effects of sewage upon the acidity or alkalinity of natural bodies of water must also be reconsidered in the light of its possible in- jurious or beneficial effects due to its chemical reaction. Thus a large number of interesting and important questions suggest themselves. The effect of the chemical reaction of the water upon the distribution of organisms promises much room for investigation. There is no doubt but that fishes recognize the difference be- tween very faintly acid or very faintly alkaline, and neutral water. Henderson's work ('13), upon the mechanism which maintains a constant proportion of H and OH ions in the blood of animals, suggests the physiological reason for this extreme sensitiveness of the fishes. It is clear that even very small variations in the proportions of these two ions in the blood of the organism, are of grave importance, and we find in the blood a combination of gases and salts that makes such variations impossible as long as the animal is normal. The blood will main- tain its normal chemical reaction (just on the alkaline side of neutrality) in the face of relatively large changes in the environ- ment, yet we know that the mechanism breaks down when the change is either too great or too long continued (acclimatization is not considered at this time). The hyper-sensitiveness of the animals to the chemical reaction of the water, in the case of aqua- tic organisms, is another important factor in preserving the normal reaction of the blood, as the reactions of the organisms work in a way that causes them to turn back from concentrations of H or OH ion that would be detrimental. The delicacy and accuracy of these reactions are evidenced in the reaction ex- periments which have been discussed in the preceding pages. The physiological effect of the acid, neutral, and alkaline water upon the organism very probably has to do with decrease or increase in the permeability of the exposed tissue cells (es- REACTION AND RESISTANCE OF FISHES. 245 pecially gills in case of fishes). Osterhout ('14) has shown that in plant cells alkalies increase the permeability up to death; acids however at first produce a rapid decrease in permeability, followed later by an increase which continues up to death. The concentrations of acid used by Osterhout were .001 .V to .03 N. Very low concentrations such as those used in the experiments discussed here would very likely maintain a permanent decrease in the permeability of the cells, and the concentrations of acid in which the fresh-water fishes normally live, may thus protect the fishes by decreasing the permeability of their gills and pre- serving the normal reaction of the blood. Alkaline water, on the other hand, does not do this for fresh-water fishes, and they soon succumb in it. The results of Shelford and Powers ('15) indicate that the action of alkaline water upon marine fishes is to produce a normal permeability of the membranes and it may be that acid water would kill these fishes by decreasing the per- meability below normal. The effect of neutrality upon the permeability of tissues has not been worked out, so far as I am aware, but since fresh-water fishes, and probably marine fishes also, are negative to neutral water, it must be that such water exerts a marked effect upon the permeability, or some other physiological condition, in the gill membranes. The negativeness of organisms to neutral water indicates that they are either over-stimulated in such water, or under-stimulation sets up internal disturbances. Thus they may avoid it because of its non-stimulating character. It may well be that in neutral water, the normal chemical reactions do not go on, for acidity and alkalinity surpass all other con- ditions, even temperature and concentration of reacting sub- stance, in their influence upon many chemical processes. Of all the catalytic agents, H and OH ions are by far the most im- portant, and in their influence upon the stability of colloidal systems they are unapproached by other substances (Henderson, '13). Birge and Juday ('n and '12) attempt to explain the vertical distribution of the plankton in the lakes of Wisconsin and New York, upon the basis of relation to oxygen and food. This attempt has, it seems to me, met with little success, and they 246 MORRIS M. WELLS. themselves point out many contradictions. According to their idea, the plankton forms must in many instances be reacting positively to concentrations of oxygen which are as small as .1 c.c. per liter, or even less. This supposition is contrary to all the experimental evidence regarding the reactions of aerobic fresh-water organisms to this gas. In an attempt to correlate the distribution of the zooplankton with the chemical reaction of the water, I have gone over Birge and Juday's tables and figures, and have come to the conclusion that such correlation exists. Their data indicate in practically all of the lakes (in the summer condition) a point at some depth below the surface of the lake, where the organisms are more numerous than at any other depth. In many cases this rise is proportionately very high and is usually of small amplitude. Thus the large number of forms occurs in a rather limited region vertically. After the rise, there is a marked diminution in the number of forms and then again at a little greater depth there is another increase, smaller than the first, but still very noticeable in their curves. This increase is followed by a second diminution. The first diminution usually occurs in or near the thermocline where the temperature often shows a very sudden lowering. The oxygen supply sometimes falls off here also, but not always, and in the lakes to which I refer particularly, the oxygen supply is practically the same at all depths. A very important fact, however, is that the water in the region of the thermocline, i. e., at the region of smallest numbers of plankton, is often neutral or very nearly so (summer condition). Above this region the water is alkaline, and below acid. From the data given in Birge and Juday's Tables XVIII. and XIX. ('12, pp. 602-608), I have compiled the following table (Table IV.) to show the re- lation of the zooplankton to this neutral region. Birge and Juday's Table XVIII. is a record of temperatures and gas con- tents at the different depths; Table XIX. is an analysis of the plankton catches made in ten lakes. The records for a given lake were all made on the same day. Table XVI 1 1 . gives titration records which show that in three of the lakes at a definite depth, the water was neutral. Table XIX. gives the plankton col- lections at different depths in these three lakes, on the same REACTION AND RESISTANCE OF FISHES. 247 day. Table IV. inserted below, is made up from a combination of the data found in the two tables; most of the data in Table IV. refer to the three lakes in question. In the instance of Triarthra, however, the data come from two other lakes, as this form does not occur in the three lakes from which the other data are taken. TABLE IV. SHOWING THE RELATION OF ZOOPLANKTON TO NEUTRALITY. The table is compiled from Birge and Juday's ('12) Tables XVIII and XIX, Most of the data are taken from their records for the three lakes, Canandaigua, Seneca, and Skaneateles. In these lakes the neutral depth was accurately located by titration of samples. The titrations and plankton collections were made on the same say. The data for the rotifer triarthra are taken from lakes Hemlock and Keuka as these are the only lakes in which this form occurs in the records, n.c. = no collection at this degree of acidity or alkalinity. The figures in the columns indicate the number of forms per cubic meter of water. Name of Animal. Alkalinity in C.c. per Liter of CO2 to Make Neutral. Neutral- ity. Acidity Ln C c. of CO2 per Liter. 3-2- 1.5-1. •5-.2S. o. .25-5- •75-1- .1-1.5. Pleosoma (R) 3.925 12,250 11,320 43-700 2,885 28,150 7.850 130 4.0OO 13-775 625 1,260 52,330 12,350 0 0 o o 400 19.130 2,750 28,050 6,660 290 1,250 7,620 685 650 104,500 1,620 n.c. n.c. 0 O O 42,8OO n.c. 13.250 17.350 250 200 7,62O 65 4OO 85,160 2,350 77O,40O O O 0 0 0 260 570 2,220 250 30 25 O 130 2,025 160 95,600 n.c. O 0 o o o 140 1,440 30 20 30 65 1.145 11,760 1,190 1,900 I.O=;O O O O O O 40 390 65 20 o 0 25 5.750 1,240 o I. IIO o o o 0 o 205 IOO 30 20 5 o o 1,670 40 1,900 2.4.2=; Vorticella (P) Asplanchna (R) ...... Dinobryon (P) Diaphanosoma (C). . . Nauplei Diaptomus (Co) Conochilus (R) Anuroea (P) Cyclops (Co) Notholca (R) Daphnia (C) Ceratium (P) Polyarthra (R) Malamonas (P) Triarthra (R) . Letters in parenthesis after name of animal indicate the following. Rotifer; (P) = Protozoan; (C) = Cladoceran; (Co) = Copepod. (R) = Table IV. shows (i) that all the zooplankton forms are more numerous on either the acid or the alkaline side of neutrality, than they are at neutrality itself, i. e., they are negative to neu- trality; (2) some forms as Pleosoma and Vorticella, are found only in the alkaline water; (3) others range between slight alka- linity and slight acidity but are never very numerous at neu- trality and often (Daphnia, Ceratium, etc.) show an increase •on either side; (4) a few forms (Triarthra) occur wholly on the acid side of neutrality. 248 MORRIS M. WELLS. The factors that regulate the distribution of the plankton in the lakes are undoubtedly numerous. The only certain way to determine them is to investigate experimentally the reactions of the animals to the factors concerned, both singly and in com- bination. To do this would be tedious but not especially dim- cult. As an index to the distribution of these forms, I believe that the presence and position of a neutral layer of water will be found to be important. Besides the experimental data presented in the papers by Birge and Juday, the literature contains much other experimental evidence which bears directly upon the question of the toxicity of neutrality to organisms. Much of this evidence is found in connection with experiments upon the toxic effects of distilled water, and the action of salts in antagonizing this toxicity. In a series of papers published by Ringer and his students between the years 1883 and 1893 the question of the toxicity of distilled water was investigated and its reality apparently demonstrated. It was also shown that various salts are effective in neutralizing this toxicity, some being much more efficacious than others. In 1893 Naegeli showed that for Algae (Spirogyra) at least, the toxicity of distilled water was due to contamination from the copper stills in which it was prepared. Locke ('95) con- firmed Naegeli's results by showing the effect upon certain fresh-water animals to be due also to the minute amounts of copper present, and Ringer ('97) again taking up the subject reversed his former conclusions and confirmed those of Locke. Jennings ('97) found that Paramcecia live for weeks in distilled water. Moore ('oo) says that young trout and tadpoles (unfed) live as long in distilled as in tap water, i. e., several weeks. Lillie ('oo) says that Planaria maculata will live in distilled water. Pure distilled water seemed then not to be toxic to fresh-water animals though apparently toxic to most marine animals. Fun- dulus eggs seem to be an exception among marine animals (Loeb, '99), as they can live in distilled water for weeks and still produce normal embryos. In 1903 Bullot after testing the effects of distilled water upon the fresh-water amphipod, Gammarus concluded that pure distilled water was toxic to this crustacean. Bullet's experiments were performed with great REACTION AND RESISTANCE OF FISHES. 249 care; he considered and seemed to have eliminated the following possible toxic factors: copper, impurities from the glass, low oxygen, ammonia, and carbon dioxide. He found also that NaCl in small concentrations would neutralize the toxicity of the pure water to such an extent that the animals lived almost as well in an .00008 N solution of this salt as in the natural fresh water. The toxicity of pure distilled water, he concluded, is due to the lack of salts in solution. Peters in 1908 performed some very careful experiments to test the effect of pure distilled water upon protozoa. He came to the conclusion that distilled water which contains no salts, and which is changed often enough to prevent their accumulation from the metabolism of the ani- mals, is rapidly toxic to these forms. I have gone over the above papers and have found many statements which indicate that the presence of a certain con- centration of hydrogen ion, was beneficial to the animals ex- perimented upon. For instance, Ringer ('83) states that the distilled water which he used killed minnows, on an average, in 4.5 hours. He also says that the distilled water was very faintly acid; so faint was the acidity that he did not rely upon his own judgment but had others make the test also. However, he says, to prove that the acidity was not the cause of the death of the minnows, he took three liters of water and to one added 6 drops of 10 per cent, acetic acid, to the next 12 drops and to the third 20 drops. He then placed three minnows in each liter of acid solution. After 24 hours, the minnowrs were "quite natural" and he concluded, therefore, that the acidity could not have been harmful in the case of the distilled water. This conclusion of Ringer's illustrates the attitude taken by most authors with regard to the presence of acid in the water, that is, the acid is looked upon as a detrimental factor, to be considered negatively. So far as I have been able to read, the authors quoted above have taken little consideration of the possibility that the presence of a certain concentration of H or OH ion is essential to the welfare of animals. This Ringer does not suggest, even though the minnows in the acid water were well on the way to live as long as any of the animals kept in salt solutions. In this same paper, Ringer notes 250 MORRIS M. WELLS. that when he put a large number of fishes (up to a maximum) into a given volume of distilled water, they lived longer than one or two fishes placed in the same volume of water. He at- tributes this to the excretion by the fishes of inorganic salts, and does not take into consideration the carbon dioxide factor which would have increased the acidity of the water to many times that of the almost neutral distilled water. Again, in speaking of the salts which are best for preserving life in distilled water, Ringer states that the calcium salts are better than those of sodium and potassium, that CaSo4 is better than CaCb and that the phosphate of lime (Ca3PO4)2 is much superior to all the other salts. This latter salt, he states, is decidedly acid, and he says ('86) "it is interesting to observe that though the circulating fluid with phosphate of lime gives a slight acid reaction to delicate blue litmus paper it will sustain contractility of muscle for hours." Thus a small hydrogen ion concentration seems to be beneficial, if not essential, to the continued life and activity of the organisms and tissues in question. The question of the existence of a carbon dioxide optimum for animals has received considerable investigation with varying results. Ringer in 1893 investigated the influence of carbonic acid upon the frog's heart and concluded that free CO^ in saline solution arrests cardiac contractility. He does not state what concentrations of CO2 were used, but since he speaks of passing carbonic acid through the solution "for some time," his solutions were probably very acid. In a few experiments he neutralized the slightly acid distilled water which was used to make up the saline solutions, with NaOH, and noted that in this neutral solution, the contractions of the heart very soon became ab- normal. Jerusalem and Starling ('10) review the literature regarding the importance of carbon dioxide for the ordinary functions of the body, and report a series of experiments to determine its influence upon the beat of the heart of the frog, tortoise and mammal (cat). They conclude that the CO2 tension in the blood must be maintained at a certain height, if the pump- ing action of the heart is to be normally carried out. In their review of the literature they point out that their conclusions are in accord with those of Miescher, Haldane, Mosso, Hender- REACTION AND RESISTANCE OF FISHES. 25! son, and Bottazzi (see pp. 279-280). The lowest concentration which Jerusalem and Starling used was 2 per cent, of an atmos- phere or about 20 c.c. CO2 per liter. Their highest ran up to 200 c.c. per liter. Hooker ('12) tested the effect of carbon dioxide upon muscular tone and, in opposition to Jerusalem and Starling, concluded that this gas does not appear to be directly beneficial to tissues, except in case of intestinal muscle rhythm. He thinks it may be indirectly beneficial. Like most other workers upon this problem, Hooker used very high concentra- tions of the gas. His concentrations varied from 5 per cent, to 20 per cent, of the gas, in the atmosphere to which the solution bathing the tissue was exposed. Water will dissolve nearly its own volume of CQ2 and thus the concentration of carbonic acid varied from 50 to 200 c.c. of CO2 per liter. The smallest concentration used would kill most fresh-water fishes in a short time. Reuss ('10) worked upon the effect of CO2 upon the respira- tion of fishes and concluded that it is an important one. The regulation according to him is through the respiratory center and not peripheral as Bethe ('03) believed. Shelford and Allee ('130) note the extreme sensitiveness of fishes to COo in gradients, and think the production of the gas as a product of the metabolism of the organism may tend to increase its ex- ternal effect when the fishes come in contact with water con- taining it. Bullot ('04) in his work with the fresh-water amphipod (Gam- marus) noted, as did Ringer in the case of fishes, that the animals lived longer in distilled water when a number was present in a given volume, or in other words, when the volume of water per individual was small. He says: "If the amount of water falls below a certain limit, the animals will live the longer, the smaller the amount of water, provided the quantity does not fall below a certain minimum." In Table V. I have collected Bullet's data showing this point. The table shows that the relation holds for both redistilled water and water distilled in copper alone. The length of life in the water from the copper still is proportionately shorter throughout. 252 MORRIS M. WELLS. TABLE V. SHOWING THE RESISTANCE OF AMPHIPODS IN DISTILLED WATER, WHEN EQUAL NUMBERS OF ANIMALS ARE PLACED IN DIFFERENT VOLUMES OF WATER, OR WHEN DIFFERENT NUMBERS OF ANIMALS ARE PLACED IN EQUAL VOLUMES OF WATER (COMPILED FROM BULLOT, '04, PP. 204-5). Per Cent, of Animals Alive After 2 Days. No. Animals. Volume of Water. Water. Redistilled in Glass. Distilled in Copper. I 5 10 Same throughout 45% 80% 90% Proportionately less throughout. Time Required to Kill One Half the Animals. Same throughout 5 c.c. 10 days 8 days 20 c.c. 2.5 days 1.5 days 50 c.c. 1.5 days i day IOO C.C. same same. In considering the possible importance of CO* as a factor in the toxicity of distilled water, Bullot states that the water which he used was very faintly acid to phenolphthalein, but not enough to injure the animals. He says: "We know from the works of P. Bert that, for cold-blooded animals like the frog, for instance, CC>2 is toxic only when its concentration in the air reaches 15 per cent. This corresponds to a solution of 15 per cent, of this gas by volume, as the water dissolves its own volume of COo at ordinary temperature and normal pressure. This quantity is 350 times larger than the one which could be found in the dis- tilled water." A 15 per cent, solution of CO2 means 150 c.c. per liter of water. 1/350 of this is .42 c.c. In other words the distilled water used by Bullot was practically neutral, since the amount of hydrogen ion to be obtained from so small a quantity of so little ionized an acid as carbonic acid, would be almost negligible. In the gradient experiments cited in this paper, I have shown that certain fishes are negative to so small a con- centration of CC>2 as i c.c. per liter, in preference for slightly higher concentrations. I have further shown that these fishes do not live as well in distilled water that is practically neutral, as they do in the same water made slightly (.00005 N) acid. Thus the existence of a hydrogen ion concentration optimum for these forms seems to be clearly demonstrated. REACTION AND RESISTANCE OF FISHES. 253 Peters ('08) makes no mention of the possibility of the neu- trality of the distilled water which he used, having something to do with its toxicity, yet in a previous paper ('07) he recognizes the importance of the presence of a certain concentration of hydrogen ion for the existence of certain protozoa in hay in- fusions. On page 346, he says: 'The data obtained indicate that, of the chemical conditions, the concentration of acid ... is one of the chief factors determining the biological content and history of a culture." From the data and discussion that have gone before, it seems certain that the chemical reaction of the water is a factor of marked importance in the life history of fresh-water animals. Some fresh-water forms are apparently positive to alkalinity as seen in the fresh-water lakes (Birge and Juday, loc. cit.} and others, that normally live in water that is acid with CO2 are not killed by living in alkaline water (isopods). On the other hand, many forms, and probably most of the fresh-water fishes belong here, are always found in acid water if such be available, and these forms cannot live normally in neutral to alkaline water. Shelford and Powers ('15) have shown that marine fishes select the alkaline side of neutrality in a gradient, and in this dif- ference in the behavior of the fishes, lies a key to the fundamental physiological difference in the organisms of these two habitats. Fresh-water fishes must live in the presence of an excess of hy- drogen ion if their life processes are to be carried on in normal fashion. Shelford ('14) states that the carbon dioxide content of the water over the breeding grounds of fresh-water fishes should not average more than I c.c. per liter, nor exceed 5 c.c. during the summer months. This statement is probably wrong in limiting the average to I c.c. per liter for some fishes, as the green spotted sunfish and the crappies are negative to this small concentration of CO2 showing a preference for slightly higher concentrations. Blue-gills, on the other hand, select a degree of acidity that is very little above neutrality. The CO2 concentration selected by fishes probably varies with the season, and certainly with the salt content of the water in which they live. The variations of the CO2 optimum in salt concentration will be discussed in the second paper of the series. 254 MORRIS M. WELLS. One thing is clear; the chemical reaction of the water should be known with accuracy, in all experiments with salts and gases in solution. A recognition of this fact will help to clear up some of the many contradictory results which have been obtained by various workers. It seems to be demonstrated beyond a doubt that the toxicity of distilled water is in part due to the absence or scarcity of inorganic salts, but it is also evident that the neutrality of such water may be an important factor in its toxicity. VI. GENERAL CONCLUSIONS. 1. The chemical reaction of the water is an important factor in the reactions and resistance of organisms. 2. Fresh-water fishes select slight acidity in a gradient, when the other choices are neutrality and alkalinity. They choose slight alkalinity in preference to neutrality. 3. The CO2 optimum for the different species of fishes ex- perimented upon, varies from very close to neutrality for the blue-gill, to 6 c.c. per liter for the sunfishes and crappies. This is for the November to January condition. At other seasons and in other waters, the optimum probably varies somewhat. The optimum acid concentration for fresh-water fishes in dis- tilled water is about .00005 N H2SO4. 4. The distribution of the plankton in the lakes of Wisconsin and New York (Birge and Juday, 'n and '12) shows a very in- teresting correlation with the chemical reaction of the water. There are fewer animals at neutrality than in the slightly alkaline and slightly acid water just above and below the neutral layer. Thus the forms are negative to neutrality. 5. The neutrality of distilled water is a factor to be considered in its toxicity. VII. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS AND BIBLIOGRAPHY. I am indebted to Professor V. E. Shelford for many helpful suggestions and numerous courtesies during the working out of the experimental data and preparation of the manuscript. REACTION AND RESISTANCE OF FISHES. 255 BIBLIOGRAPHY. Alice, W. C. '13 Further Studies on Physiological States and Rheotaxis in Isopoda. Jour. Expt. Zool., Vol. 15, No. 3. Birge, E. A. and Juday, C. 'u The Inland Lakes of Wisconsin. The Dissolved Gases of the Water and their Biological Significance. Wis. Geol. Nat. Hist. Surv., Bull. No. 22. Sc. Series, Vol. 7. '12 A Limnological Study of the Finger Lakes of New York. Bull. Bur. Fisheries, Vol. 32, Doc. No. 791. Bullot, G. '04 On the Toxicity of Distilled Water for Fresh-water Gammanis. Suppres- sion of This Toxicity by the Addition of Small Quantities of NaCl. Univ. Calif. Publ. Physiol., Berkeley, Vol. i, pp. 199-217. Henderson, Lawrence J. '13 The Fitness of the Environment. Macmillan Co., New York. Herrick, C. Judson. '02 The Organ and Sense of Taste in Fishes. Bull. U. S. F. C., Vol. 22, p. 239- 271. Hooker, D. R. '12 The Effect of Carbon Dioxide and Oxygen upon Muscular Tone in the Blood-vessels and Alimentary-canal. Am. Jour. Physiol., Vol. 31, p. 47. Jennings, H. '97 Studies on Reactions to Stimuli in Unicellular Organisms. I. Reactions to Chemical, Osmotic, and Mechanical Stimuli in the Ciliate Infusoria. Jour, of Physiol., Vol. 21, p. 258, 64 p. Jerusalem, R. and Starling, E. H. '10 On the Significance of Carbon Dioxide for the Heart Beat. Jour. Phys. Lond., Vol. XL., No. 4. Lillie, F. R. 'oo Some Notes on Regeneration and Regulation in Planarians. The American Naturalist, Vol. 34, 5 p. Locke, S. '95 On a Supposed Action of Distilled Water as Such, on Certain Animal Organisms. Jour. Physiol., Vol. 18, p. 318. Loeb, J. '99 On lon-proteid Compounds and their Role in the Mechanics of Life Phe- nomena. I. The Poisonous Character of a Pure NaCl Solution. Am. Jour. Physiol., Vol. 3, p. 327, n p. Marsh, M. C. '06 The Effect of some Industrial Wastes on Fishes. House Documents, Vol. 64, Water Supply and Irrigation Papers. Moore, A. 'oo Further Evidence of the Poisonous Effects of a Pure NaCl Solution. Am. Jour. Physiol., Vol. 4, p. 386. Naegeli, C. '93 Ueber oligodynamische Erscheinungen in lebenden Zellen mit einem Vorwort von S. Schwendener und einem Nachtrag von C. Cramer. Denk- schriften d. Schweiz. Naturforsch. Gesellsch., Bd. 33, 1893. 256 MORRIS M. WELLS. Noyes, W. A. '13 A Text-book of Chemistry. Henry Holt Company, New York. Osterhout, W. J. V. '14 The Effect of Alkali on Permeability. Jour. Biol. Chemistry, Vol. 19, p. 335- '14 The Effect of Acid on Permeability. Jour. Biol. Chemistry, Vol. 19, p. 493- Peters, A. W. '07 Chemical Studies on the Cell and Its Medium. II. Some Chemico- biological Relations in Liquid Culture Media. Am. Jour. Physiol., Vol. 18, p. 321. '08 Chemical Studies on the Cell and Its Medium. III. The Function of the Inorganic Salts of the Protozoan Cell and Its Medium. Am. Jour. Physiol., Vol. 21, pp. 105-124. Powers, E. B. '14 The Reactions of Cray-fishes to Gradients of Dissolved Carbon Dioxide and Acetic and Hydrochloric Acids. BIOL. BULL., Vol. 27, No. 4, pp. 177-199. Reuss, H. '10 Die Wirkung der Kohlensaure auf Atmung der Niederen Wirbeltiere in besonderen der Fische. Zeit. fur Biol., Vol. 53, pp. 555-587. Ringer, Sidney. '83 Concerning the Influence of Saline Media on Fish. Jour. Physiol., Vol. 5, p. 98. '84 Regarding the Influence of the Organic Constituents of the Blood on the Contractility of the Ventricle. Jour. Physiol., Vol. 6, p. 361. '86 Further Experiments Regarding the Influence of Lime, Potassium and Other Salts on Muscular Tissue. Jour. Physiol., Vol. 7, p. 290. '93 The Influence of Carbonic Acid Dissolved in Saline Solutions, on the Ventricle of the Frog's Heart. Jour. Physiol., Vol. 14, p. 125. '97 The Action of Distilled Water on Tubifex. Jour. Physiol., Vol. 22, Proc. of Physiol. Soc. Ringer, S. and Buxton, D. W. '84 Concerning the Action of Small Quantities of Ca, Na, and K Salts upon the Vitality and Function of Contractile Tissues and the Cuticuler Cells of Fishes. Jour. Physiol., Vol. 6, p. 154. Seyler, C. A. '94 Notes on Water Analysis. Chemical News, Vol. 70, p. 104. Shelford, V. E. '14 Suggestions as to Indices of the Suitability of Bodies of Water for Fishes. Transactions Am. Fisheries Soc., Dec., 1914. Shelford, V. E. and Alice, W. C. '13 The Reactions of Fishes to Gradients of Dissolved Atmospheric Gases. Jour. Expt. Zool., Vol. 14, No. 2, Feb., 1913. '14 Rapid Modification of Behavior of Fishes by Contact with Modified Water. Jour. Animal Behavior, Vol. 4, No. i, pp. 1-30. Shelford, V. E. and Powers, E. B. '15 An Experimental Study of the Movements of Herring and Other Marine Fishes. Biol. Bull., Vol. 28, No. 5, pp. 315-334. REACTION AND RESISTANCE OF FISHES. 257 Stieglitz, Julius. '09 The Relation of Equilibrium between the Carbon Dioxide of the Atmosphere and the Calcium Sulphate, Calcium Carbonate, and Calcium Bicarbonate of Water Solutions in Contact with It. Carnegie Inst. of Wash. Pub. No. 107, p. 235. Traube-Mengarini, M. and Scala, A. '12 Die Wirkung des reinen und des elektrolythaltigen destillierten Wassers auf Metalle. Zeitschrift fur Chemie und Industrie der Kolloide, Bd. 10, p. 114-119. Wells, M. M. '13 The Resistance of Fishes to Different Concentrations and Combinations of Carbon Dioxide and Oxygen. BIOL. BULL..VO!. 25, No. 6. '14 The Reactions and Resistance of Fishes to Temperature. Transactions 111. Acad. of Science, Vol. VII., p. . A PROCESS OF TEMPORARY CHAIN FORMATION BY FRONTONIA.1 WALDO SHUMWAY. In May of this year, while studying the protozoan fauna of a small pond in the New York Botanical Gardens, the writer observed what appeared to be a chain of four holophrya-like ciliates. A careful search of the culture made during the fol- lowing two weeks revealed about a dozen more such chains before the species disappeared. During this time the writer attempted to obtain a pure culture of these interesting forms, with an idea to studying the nuclear changes involved. Although these attempts were all, ultimately, unsuccessful, it has seemed best to put on record what few facts have been observed. The data here given has been obtained from the study of the living material and some few preparations both in toto and sectioned. The following stages in the formation of these chains have been observed; single individuals, two-cell chains and four-cell chains. No chains of three or more than four cells have been observed among the large number (over one hundred and fifty) observed. The solitary individual is an exceedingly large (circa 300 micra) ovoid holotrichous ciliate, densely pigmented and filled with large alveoli, of some alloplasmic substance which stains deeply with nuclear dyes. The color is dark brown to black with transmitted light and light brown to white with reflected light. The mouth is anterior and lateral with rows of cilia which simulate two undulating membranes. There is a large lateral contracting vacuole. The cortical layer contains trichocysts. The macronucleus is large and oval, the micronuclei have not been observed in my material. The form is an active swimmer in the surface film of a large culture, but when isolated in Syra- cuse watch glasses sinks to the bottom and encysts. Details of this process are given below. In cases where the individual neither dies nor encysts after 1 From the Zoological Laboratory, Columbia University. 258 CHAIN FORMATION BY FRONTONIA. 259 isolation, it becomes transformed by clearing of the pigment, lengthening and flattening the general shape of the body until it resembles the well-known species Frontonia leucas. To this species, therefore, I have assigned my material, although I am not positive whether what I have observed is a new stage in the life-cycle of Frontonia leucas or a new species. The process of chain formation is inaugurated by the formation of a large transparent cyst through the exudation of some gelati- nous material in which the surrounding zooglcea becomes en- tangled in large quantities. Within this cyst the individual slowly rotates, all the cilia beating and the contractile vacuole pulsating regularly. In one individual followed under the mic- roscope a single transverse division occured about forty-five minutes after the beginning of encystment, and half an hour from its completion. Nine hours after, the daughter cells were dead, still connected and inside the cyst wall. FIG. i. Chain of four cells, Frontonia leucas. 200 X. Outline drawn with camera from preparation fixed with sublimate-acetic and stained with picro- carmine. Details restored from free-hand drawings of living material. Another encysted individual which had been isolated at the same time but which unfortunately had not been followed under the microscope had formed a chain of four cells. The writer has at different times observed in these cysts, single, double, and quadruple forms, as well as cysts containing two or four separate cells. One chain of four individuals was removed from its cyst for observation: it swam about for a time resembling an animated chain of beads, but finally broke up into its four 26O WALDO SHUMWAY. constituent parts, which shortly assumed the typical Fron- tonia leucas appearance. While the writer has not actually observed the process of transformation from the two-cell to the four-cell stage assumed above, he feels convinced from the facts cited as well as from sectioned material in which the macronuclei of the two anterior and the two posterior cells seem to have just undergone division, that these chains are formed by two successive transverse divis- sions without separation of the daughter cells and not by a single quadrupartite or by three successive terminal divisions as might be argued from a priori grounds. Division preceded by encystment is not unknown among the free-swimming ciliates. In Otostoma carteri and Ampliilep- tus meleagris according to Saville-Kent the divisions are some- times multiple, but the daughter cells separate immediately after each division. Chain formation too is not unknown among the ciliates, although heretofore it has been observed only among the asto- matous forms parasitic in the digestive tracts of vertebrates. In these parasites, however, the chains are often of great length and are formed, so the weight of evidence appears, by a process of terminal budding. For an excellent discussion of these forms see Cepede (1910). Jennings ('08) records a strain of Paramcecia appearing in his cultures which had apparently lost the power of separation after division. The general appearance and weak vitality of these forms, however point to the conclusion that Jennings was dealing with a weakened pathological race and not a genetic mutation. The frequency with which these chains appeared, nearly ten per cent, of all the Frontonia observed, leads the writer to conclude that the phenomena described in this paper form part of a normal method of reproduction. I have made attempts to raise these forms on hay infusion, rotten lettuce infusion (recommended by Popoff, '08), thyroid extract and the filtered water of the medium in which they were discovered. All were unsuccessful. I have been unable to maintain isolated individuals in Syracuse glasses of their CHAIN FORMATION BY FRONTONIA. 26 1 normal medium longer than two days. For this reason I am unable to give a fuller account of the process under discussion nor carry out the experimental work suggested by it. The case however appears to be a unique instance of normal temporary chain formation among the free-living ciliates. LITERATURE CITED. Cepede, C. '10 Recherches sur les Infusoires astomes. Arch, de Zool. Exp., 5 ser., III., 4, 1910. Jennings, H. S. '08 Heredity, Variation and Evolution in Protozoa. Jour. Exp. Zool., V., 4, 1908. Popoff, M. '08 Experimented Zellstudien. Arch. f. Zellforsch., I., 2, 1908. Saville Kent, W. '81 Manual of the Infusoria, 1881-2. SPERMATOGENESIS IN PARATETTIX.1 MARY T. HARMAN. Wilson has said that "heredrity is a consequence of the genetic continuity of cells by division, and the germ cells form the vehicle of transmission from one generation to another." If this be true we should look to the structure of the germ-cells fo • an explanation of the phenomena that have been and are being found out in heredity. Cytologists have discovered much concerning the structure of the germ-cells and the behavior of the chromosomes during the processes of maturation and division. The combined knowledge of sex and sex ratio, and the cytologi- cal constitution of germ-cells has shown in many forms, at least, a correlation between the inheritance of sex and the dimorphism of spermatozoa or eggs, or both. However, the vast amount of cytological work has been done with forms the behavior of whose characteristics in heredity is unknown. On the other hand, much of the work in heredity has been done with forms of which little or nothing is known of the structure of the germ- cells. It is the writer's good fortune to have access to material of which some of the ancestry is known for eighteen generations, covering a period of five years. For a number of years Dr. R. K. Nabours2 has been conducting experiments with regard to inheritance in Paratettix, a genus of the short-horned grasshoppers. The characteristics used in his investigations are the color patterns of the pronotum and femora of the jumping legs, and the lengths of the pronotum and wings. The data show that the inheritance of the color patterns is Mendelian in its behavior. In the FI hybrid no part of the color pattern of one parent species is ever replaced by the color pattern of the other parent, but the color patterns of both parents are present. Reciprocal crosses give identical results. The 1 Contribution from the Zoological Laboratory, Kansas State Agricultural College, No. 7. 2 The writer wishes to thank Dr. R. K. Nabours for the grasshoppers which have furnished the material for this paper. 262 SPERMATOGENESIS IN PARATETTIX. 263 lengths of the wings and pronotum are not inherited but are closely correlated with the length of time required for the animal to reach maturity. These grasshoppers have furnished the material for the present paper. The work on the cytological constitution of the germ-cells of Paratettix has been undertaken for the purpose of discovering whether or not the microscope will reveal any differences in the germ-cells of very closely related forms which may be corre- lated with the differences in the color pattern. The spermato- genesis of only one form (Paratettix leuconotus-leucothorax} is given here. Paratettix leuconotus-leucothorax is a hybrid, obtained by crossing P. leuconottis with P. leucothorax (or by the interbreeding of two hybrids, one being a hybrid of leuconotus with some other form and the other a hybrid of leucothorax with some other form). No attempt has been made to show any relation between the structure of the germ-cells and the somatic structures. This will be discussed in a later paper. The chromosomal complex of the spermatogonia of Para- tettix leuconotus-leucothorax consists of thirteen rod-shaped bodies which may be divided into two groups — one group con- sisting of four larger chromosomes and the other of nine smaller ones. Neither the larger nor the smaller chromosomes form equal sized pairs as Sutton has found in Brachystola magna and which is so frequently described for the Hemiptera and is ap- parently characteristic of all Diptera. All of the large chromo- somes and one of the small ones are bent rods or slightly U- shaped, but the other eight are almost straight. No one of these chromosomes has been surely identified as the accessory. However, in the early prophases there is always present a mass of chromatin which has a more compact consistency and stains more intensely than the remainder of the chromatin (A, Fig. i). This mass has not been identified with any chromosome nor is it associated with a vesicle as described by Carothers for Arphia simplex. There appears to be no difference in the staining capacity nor in the compactness of the chromosomes in the late prophases (Fig. 2). The spermatogonial spindle is long and slender, and has fine but distinct fibers which converge at the poles. The centrosome 264 MARY T. HARM AN. which is very distinctly visible in the metaphase stage is small and spherical. It stains almost as intensely as the chromosomes. The astral rays are short and indistinct. The chromosomes are at right angles to the spindle fibers in the metaphase stage (Fig. 7). A metaphase plate always shows one chromosome nearer the center of the spindle than any other chromosome. Sometimes it is completely surrounded by the others (Figs. 4 and 6) and sometimes merely one end is at the center of the spindle (Figs. 3 and 5). This chromosome is always one of the larger of the group of smaller ones but it is never the bent one. Few anaphase and no telophaase stages have been observed. Fig. 8 shows an anaphase with rather indistinct spindle fibers, which is characteristic of all the anaphase stages observed. The cen- trosome, which shows distinctly in the metaphase stage (Fig. 7), is now invisible. The chromasomes are no longer at right angles to the spindle fibers but are nearly parallel with them. Fig. 9 illustrates the condition of the cell at the beginning of the growth period. The nucleus is large and comparatively clear. Some of the chromatin is in a finely reticular condition and stains faintly with iron-hsematoxylin. However, a mass of the chromatin retains the compact consistency and the density of stain of the chromosomes (A, Fig. 9). It has a rounded form like a nucleolus. The boundary between the nucleus and the cytoplasm is quite evident. The nucleus continues to increase in size and the reticular chromatin, which now has a greater staining capacity, forms a thread or threads having a woolly appearance. There is no polarization of the loops of the spireme but they occupy almost all of the space of the nucleus and form an irregular tangled mass. The nucleolus remains at one side of the nucleus and does not have the woolly appearance that the spireme has (Fig. 10). In the synezesis or contraction stage, the spireme seems to shrink away from the nuclear wall, leaving a clear space between the cytoplasm and the chromatin material. There is little difference between the character of the chromatin and its staining capacity in this stage and the preceding one. The compact mass of chromatin never loses its identity and always remains at one side of the nucleus (Fig. n). The boundary of SPERMATOGENESIS IN PARATETTIX. 265 the nucleus soon becomes irregular, and the chromosomes of the primary spermatocyte is formed by a breaking up of the spireme thread into segments. The compact intensely staining mass which has been traced through the growth period is shown in Fig. 12 as a chromosome which differs in shape from the other chromosomes. It is ovoid and without a constriction in the middle, while all the other chromosomes are dumb-bell shaped. Not all of the chromosomes are formed at the same time. The chromatin retains its loose woolly appearance, until after it has broken up into parts, then it gradually becomes more compact, takes the stain more readily and each part assumes the characteristic dumb-bell shape. While this is taking place the boundary between the nucleus and the cytoplasm becomes more irregular and by the time the chromosomes are completely formed the cytoplasm has formed a vesicle around each of them (Figs. 13 and 14). The chromosomal complex of the primary spermatocyte con- sists of six dumb-bell shaped chromosomes and one ovoid chro- mosome. Of the six dumb-bell chromosomes two are decidedly larger than the others and one of these is much larger than the other one, as is shown in Figs. 13 to 16 inclusive. The ovoid or accessory chromosome is never among the other chromosomes but always lies near the periphery of the nucleus as it did in the growth period. When the chromosomes have become arranged on the spindle the dumb-bell chromosomes are well toward the center of the spindle, while the accessory is always near the periphery. It does not remain long in the metaphase plate but soon passes toward one pole undivided much in advance of the other chromosomes. For this reason many sections of metaphase plates show only six chromosomes and those which show seven are often cut obliquely. Not all of the chromosomes in the primary spermatocyte divide synchronously. Fig. 20 shows five of the dumb-bell chromosomes divided while the largest one shows little constriction. This division is transverse as is shown in Figs. 16 and 20. There are no loops, rings, or U's which would give the least indication of a longitudinal division. In the metaphase or early anaphases the centrosome is a small spherical body and takes the stain readily. The 266 MARY T. HARMAN. spindle fibers are fine but distinct. The astral rays are similar to those of the spermatogonial divisions. In the late anaphases the centrosome is no longer visible and the spindle fibers are indistinct. There seems to be no resting stage between the first maturation division and the second maturation division. The chromosomes of the second spermatocyte are ovoid. Metaphase plates show six and seven chromosomes (Figs. 23 and 24. The accessory cannot always be distinguished from the other chromosomes. It is either the second or the third largest. It divides in this division and passes to the poles in advance of the others (Fig. 25). In the late anaphases all the chromosomes have coalesced, although the number may yet be distinguished (Fig. 26). By the time the chromosomes have reached the poles they form a diffuse mass of chromatin at each pole, and the cell has begun to constrict in the middle. The centrosome which is similar to the centrosome of the primary spermatocyte has disappeared. The spindle fibers have become indistinct. As the constriction of the cell is completed the chromatin has migrated to the center of each daughter cell. In the changing of the spermatid into the spermatozoon two things are conspicuous from the beginning, the changes in the character of the chromatin and the elongation of the cell body. From the diffuse irregular mass there is formed an ovate body with the chromatin in a coarsely reticular condition largely around the periphery of it. The cell becomes elongate and larger at one end than at the other. The cytoplasm has changed from the tangled network to fibrillar strands of granules extending longitudinally across the cell. The cell wall is quite distinct. This condition is illustrated in Fig. 29. The spermatid continues to elongate. The nucleus becomes spherical and remains at one end of the spermatid. The more granular cytoplasm lies toward the periphery of the tail-like elongation. There is a portion of the cytoplasm extending from the nucleus through the center of the tail wilich is more finely granular than the remainder and takes the stain less readily. Part of the chromatin has become massed together, forming a sphere situated to the side of the nucleus near the end of the lightly staining area of the tail. The greater part of the remaining chromatin is dis- SPERMATOGENESIS IN PARATETTIX. 267 tributed around the periphery of the nucleus (Fig. 30). As the tail becomes longer it becomes thinner and a filament extends from the nodule of the head through the entire length of the tail. Very little cytoplasm now surrounds the head (Fig. 31). Finally the head becomes arrow shaped and stains very intensely. The tail is long and filamentous and stains a little less intensely than the head. The head and a portion of the tail are illustrated by Fig. 32. The tail is more than four times as long as is shown in the figure. McClung ('14) says: "It seems very evident that in the spermatogonia of the Acrididae we are dealing with a chromo- some complex of a very definite and precise organization which, in the great majority, presents itself without essential variation in number, size and form, fiber attachment, arrangement in the metaphase and behavior during division of its elements. Steno- bothrus and Pamphagns seem to be definite exceptions in some of these respects. . . . And again he says: "With the exception of the Stenobothrus-like species, and Pamphagns, the students of the Acrididae have reported a reduc- tion of the 23 spermatogonial chromosomes to 12." All of the genera of the family Acrididae1 discussed in McClung's paper belong to the three subfamilies, Tryxalinae, (Edipodinae, and Acridinee, and none belong to the subfamily Tettiginae. It would seem that with the general agreement of the great numbers of genera of this family which he and his students have studied as well as those of other independent workers that he would be justified in making the general statements concerning the family. However, Paratettix leuconotus-leucotliorax of the subfamily Tettiginae, show some exceptions. The spermato- gonial number of chromosomes are thirteen instead of twenty- three. The number of chromosomes in the primary spermato- cyte is seven. This the writer has found to be true also for both the parent forms of the hybrid as well as for others belonging to the genus Paratettix. 1 The writer is aware of the fact that there has been much shifting about of names of the short-horned grasshoppers and that some taxonomists consider the the grouse locusts of family value. If this should be the position which McClung takes, then he would not consider Paratettix as belonging to the family Acrididae and it would follow that the observations recorded in this paper would not be excep- tions to his statements concerning Acrididae. 268 MARY T. HARMAN. Robertson ('15) says: "In the Tettigidae (Tettiginse) a sub- family of the short-horned grasshopper family Acrididse, I have found for all the specimens of at least four different genera which I have examined the number of chromosomes to be uniformly 14 in the female and 13 in the male." From the above data one would scarcely be justified in saying that the characteristic number of spermatogonial chromosomes of the subfamily Tettiginae is thirteen but the writer feels justified in saying that this is an essential variation in the number of chromosomes given in the above quotation from McClung as the number characteristic for the family Acrididae. The writer has found no indication of multiple chromosomes. In the prophase tetrad six of the chromosomes are always dumb-bell-shaped and one ovoid. There are none of the irregu- lar shaped chromosomes as described by McClung and no in- dications of the annular chromosomes which he says "that practically without exception every investigator of recent years who has made a careful study of the maturation stages in the Orthoptera has seen and figured." If the dumb-bell-shaped chromosomes are similar to his I-shaped chromosomes they differ in that they do not have an enlargement in the middle, but rather they have the appearance of a constriction. This constriction is not due to the initiation of the division, for it is present before the chromosomes are arranged on the spindle; in fact, they have their characteristic shape before the spindle is visible. The presence of a mass of chromatin in the resting stage of the spermatogonial divisions which is of a different form and different staining capacity and also the presence of a similar mass in the growth period, which can be identified as the accessory chromosome of the spermatocyte, gives added evidence for the continuity of chromosomes as definite entities. The spermatozoon of Paratettix leuconotus-leucothorax is very different from the spermatozoon of Paratettix cuculatus as described by Hancock. He describes and figures the head of P. cuculatus as being small, thin, and acutely pointed. In fact, from his figure one would think that the head is very little thicker than the tail. He says that the middle piece is formed into a SPERMATOGENESIS IN PARATETTIX. 269 high, rather short, protoplasmic keel. No keel has been observed as forming a part of the middle piece of P. leuconotiis-leucothorax. The head is many times thicker then the tail and is decidedly arrow shaped. The middle piece seems to continue from the head to the long filamentous tail without a definite dividing line between them. SUMMARY. 1. Paratettix huconotus-leucothorax has thirteen spermatogo- nial rod-shaped chromosomes, four larger and nine smaller ones. 2. Neither the larger nor the smaller chromosomes form equal sized pairs. 3. The four larger chromosomes and one of the smaller ones are bent rods, the others are almost straight. 4. Neither spermatogonial chromosome has been surely identified as the accessory chromosome. 5. In the growth period is a mass of chromatin which is more compact and stains more intensely than the remainder of the chromatin. This is the accessory chromosome of the first spermatocyte. 6. The first spermatocyte chromosomes are formed in vesicles. 7. There are six dumb-bell-shaped bivalent chromosomes and one ovoid univalent chromosome in the primary spermato- cyte. 8. The accessory is near the periphery of the nucleus and passes to one pole undivided slightly in advance of the other chromosomes in the first spermatocyte division. 9. The bivalent chromosomes divide crosswise. 10. The accessory chromosome divides in the second division. 11. The spermatozoon has an arrow-shaped head and a long filamentous tail. BIBLIOGRAPHY. Artom, C. '09 Cromosomi ed eterocromosomi nelli cinesi spermatogenetiche di Stauronotus- maroccanus Thumb. Biologica, Vol. 2. Buchner, P. '09 Das accessorische Chromosom in Spermatogenese und Oogenese der Or- thopteren, Zugleich ein Beitrag zur Kenntnis der Reduktion. Archiv fur Zellforschung, Bd. 3, Hft. 3. Brunelli, Gustavo. '10 La Spermatogenesi della Tryxalis. Division! Spermatogoniali. Memorie della Societa Italiana Scienze, Tome 16, serie 30. 27O MARY T. HARMAN. 'n La Spermatogenesi della Tryxalis. Division! maturative. Reale Academia dei Lincei, Tome 8, serie 5a. Carothers, Eleanor. '13 The Mendelian Ratio in Relation to Certain Orthopteran Chromosomes. Journ. Morph., Vol. 24, No. 4. Davis, H. S. '08 Spermatogenesis in Acrididse and Locustodae. Bull. Museum Comp. Zool., Harvard College, Vol. 43, No. 2. Gerard, Pol. '09 Recherches sur la Reduction Karyogamique dans la Spermatogenese de Stenobothrus biguttulus (L.). Bull. Societe Roy. Sciences med. nat. de Bruxelles, No. i. '09 Recherches sur la Spermatogenese chez Stenobothrus biguttulus (Linn.). Archives de Biologic, Tome 24. Granata, Leopoldo. '10 La cinesi Spermatogenetische di Pamphagus marmoratus (Brum). Archiv fur Zellforschung, Bd. 5, Hft. 2. Hancock, J. L. '02 The Tettigidae of North America. The Lakeside Press. R. R. Donneley & Sons Company, Chicago. Hartman, Frank. '13 Variations in the Size of Chromosomes. BIOL. BULL., Vol. 24, No. 4. McClung, C. E. 'oo The Spermatocyte Divisions of the Acrididae. Kansas U. Quarterly, Vol. 9, No. i. '02 The Accessory Chromosome — Sex Determinant? BIOL. BULL., Vol. 3, Nos. 1-2. '05 The Chromosome Complex of Orthopteran Spermatocytes. BIOL. BULL., Vol. 9, No. 5. '08 Cytology and Taxonomy. Kansas U. Science Bull., Vol. 4, No. 7. ' '14 A Comparative Study of the Chromosomes in Orthopteran Spermatogenesis. Journ. Morph., Vol. 25, No. 4. Meek, C. F. Y. 'n The Spermatogenesis of Stenobothrus viridulus, with Special Reference to the Heterotropic Chromosome as a Sex Determinant in Grasshoppers. Linnean Society's Journ. Zool., Vol. 32, No. 208. 'i2& A Metrical Analysis of Chromosome Complexes, showing correlation of Evolutionary Development and Chromatin Thread-width Throughout the Animal Kingdom. Philosoph. Transactions Roy. Soc. of London, Series B., Vol. 203. 7i2b The Correlation of Somatic Characters and Chromatin Rod-lengths, being a further study of Chromosome Division. Linnean Society's Journ. Zool., Vol. 32. Metz, Charles W. '14 Chromosome Studies in the Diptera. I. A Preliminary Survey of Five Different Types of Chromosome Groups in the Genus Drosophila. Journ. Exper. Zool., Vol. 17, No. i. Montgomery, T. H., Jr. '05 The Spermatogenesis of Syrbula and Lycosa, with general Considerations SPERMATOGKXKSIS IN PARATJ.TIIX. 271 upon Chromosome Reduction and the Heterochromosomes. Proc. Academy Nat. Sci. Philadelphia, February. Nabours, R. K. '14 Studies of Inheritance and Evolution in Orthoptera, I. Journ. of Genetics, Vol. 3- No. 3. Nowlin, Nadine. '08 The Chromosome Complex of Melanoplus bivitattus Say. Kansas U. Sci. Bull., Vol. 4, No. 12. Pinney, Edith. '08 Organization of the Chromosomes in Phrynotettix magnus. Kansas U. Sci. Bull., Vol. 4, No. 14. Robertson, W. R. B. '08 The Chromosome Complex of Syrbula admirabilis. Kansas U. Sci. Bull.. Vol. 4, No. 13. '15 Chromosome Studies. III. Inequalities and Deficiencies in Homologous Chromosomes; their Bearing upon Synapsis and the Loss of Unit Char- acters. Journ. Morph., Vol. 26, No. i. Wilson, E. B. "13 Heredity and Microscopical Research. Science, N.S., Vol. 37, No. 961. 272 MARY T. HARMAN. EXPLANATION OF PLATES. All figures were made at table level by means of a Zeiss compensating ocular No. 6 and a 1.5 mm. objective with the aid of a camera lucida. The drawings were enlarged two diameters and then reduced one third. PLATE I. FIG. i. Early prophase of spermatogonial division. A, a mass of chromatin which does not become reticular but remains more or less compact. FIG. 2. Formation of spermatogonial chromosomes. FIGS. 3 TO 6. Metaphase plates of spermatogonial chromosomes. FIG. 7. Metaphase, lateral view, spermatogonial division showing position of the chromosomes on the spindle. FIG. 8. Anaphase of spermatogonial division. FIG. 9. Beginning of the growth period. A, a mass of chromatin which does not pass into a reticular condition and forms the accessory chromosome. FIG. 10. Formation of chromatin thread. FIG. ii. Synizesis. FIG. 12. Beginning of the formation of the primary spermatocyte chromosomes. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXIX. PLATE I. g ,/. ..v-.>vY ,'OH, w/12 very faint (CH3),CHCH2OH, 7w/i6 + very faint (CH3)2CHCH2OH, m/20 + + (CH3)2CHCH2OH, w/24 + 4- 4- Amyl alcohol, m/2o C2H5CH3CHCH2OH, w/40. very faint1 +1 C2H5CH3CHCH2OH, wz/8o very faint very faint 4-1 C2HSCH3CHCH2OH, m/i6o faint 4-1 4- C"H5CH3CHCH2OH, ?w/32O + 4- 4- Capryl alcohol, 7^/400 CH3(CH2)6CH2OH, TW/Soo very faint very faint CH3(CH2)6CH2OH, ra/i6oo faint faint CH3(CH2)6CH2OH, wz/3200. . . . faint faint 4-1 CHsCCH-OeCHzOH, 771/6400. . + 4- 4- Sea water + 4- 4- 1 Probably due to evaporation of alcohol. the light was found to disappear almost immediately; with tannin, chloral hydrate, vanillin and sodium glycocholate the light had disappeared in the course of one hour while saponine, amygdalin, and sodium taurocholate had no effect. It is sur- prising that saponin has no effect on luminous bacteria when we consider its great cytolytic power 'on other forms in very small concentration. SUMMARY. The effects on luminous bacteria of dilution of sea water with water and m sugar solution; of HC1 and valerianic acid; of NaOH and methyl amine; of the salts of sea water in different combina- tions; and of methyl, ethyl, propyl, butyl, amyl and capryl alcohol were studied. The points of interest in the results are indicated after each table. FURTHER NOTES ON THE CHROMOSOMES OF THE CERCOPIM:. ALICE M. BORING AND RAYMOND H. FOOLER. The chromosomes in the spermatogenesis of five species of this family of Hemiptera have already been studied by Stevens1 and Boring.2 Three more species have now been studied in comparison with those previously studied. They are Philaenus lineatus, Aphrophora paralJela and Clastoptera proteus. Each of these three species belongs to a genus in which one or more species has already been studied, so this gives a chance to compare the spermatogenesis in closely related species. This has been done very carefully by McClung3 for some families of Orthoptera. The entire family Acrididae has the same spermatogonial chro- mosome number, 23, and the Locustidae has 33, but within each family there are generic and specific cytological differences. The family Cercopidae of the Hemiptera does not show as closely graded a series of cytological differences as the orthopteran families studied by McClung. The facts as found are here recorded. The material was collected at Woods Hole4 and in Orono; Philanus lineatus from grasses, Aphrophora parallela from Scotch pines, and Clastoptera proteus from alders. Dr. Herbert Osborn has very kindly identified the species of the material. Flem- ming's and Gilson's solutions were used for fixation, and iron hasmatoxylin for staining. Philcenus lineatus has 29 chromosomes as spermatogonial number (Fig. i), two of which are larger than the others. The odd chromosome is round or oval in the early (Fig. 2) as well as 1 N. M. Stevens, '06, "Studies in Spermatogenesis," Pt. II., Carnegie Institute, Washington. 2 A. M. Boring, '07, "A Study of the Spermatogenesis in the Membracidae," etc., Jour. Exp. Zool., 4, p. 469. '13, "The Chromosomes of the Cercopidae," BIOL. BULL., 24, p. 133. 3 C. E. McClung, '08, "Cytology and Taxonomy," Kans. Univ. Bull. 4. 4 We wish to thank the Director of the Marine Biological Laboratory for the privileges of the laboratory during the summers of 1913 and 1915, at which time this material was collected. 312 CHROMOSOMES OF THE CERCOPID.E. 313 the late (Fig. 3) spireme stages. The reduced number of chro- mosomes is 15 in the first spermatocytes, one of which is larger than the others (Fig. 4). The odd chromosome is univalent 6 8 9 10 11 12 14 15 16 17 - 5) and does not divide in the first spermatocyte division (Fig. 6). The second spermatocytes have partly 15 (Fig. 7) 314 ALICE M. BORING AND RAYMOND H. FOGLER. and partly 14 (Fig. 8) chromosomes. The chromosome number is specific, as the reduced number is 15, while only 12 are found in Phil&nus spumarius. But the roundness of the odd chro- mosome throughout the spireme stages is a feature common to both species of this genus, and distinguishing it from the species of the genus Aphrophora. Aphrophora parallela has 15 chromosomes as reduced number, with one largest chromosome (Fig. n). The odd chromosome is elongated in the early spireme stages (Fig. 9) and becomes more nearly round in the later stages (Fig. 10). The odd chromosome is, as usual, univalent (Fig. 12) and does not divide in the first spermatocyte division (Fig. 13). The chromosome number in the second spermatocytes is 14 and 15 (Figs. 14 and 15). Again in this species, the chromosome number is different from that in the other species of the same genus, that is, 15, in comparison with 14 in Aphrophora quadrinotata and 12 in Aphrophora spu- maria. The long odd chromosome in the early spireme stages is a common feature of both A. spumarius and A. parallela, and distinguishes them from the genera Philcemis and Clastoptera. The early spireme stages of A. quadrinotata were not studied. The species formerly classified as A . quadrangularis has since been put into the genus Lepyronia. This species does not possess the long odd chromosome characteristic of the genus Aphrophora. Clastoptera proteus has 7 as reduced chromosome number (Fig. 1 6), one less than the reduced number in Clastoptera obtusa. Unfortunately only a few stages were found in this material, so TABLE I. Genus. Species. Reduced Chromosome Number. Philaenus spumarius 12 " lineatus i S Aphrophora spumaria 12 quadrinotata 14 parallela 15 Lepyronia quadrangularis 1 1 Clastoptera proteus 7 obtusa 8 that the only other significant point that was observed was that the first spermatocyte division is the one in which the odd chro- mosome does not divide (Fig. 17) as in all the species of this family. CHROMOSOMES OF THE CERCOPID^E. 315 The eight species of Cercopida in which the spermatogenesis has so far been studied belong to four genera. The chromosome number (reduced) varies from 7 to 15. The chromosome number seems to have no significance for family or genus. The specific numbers are shown in Table I. The odd chromosome in the spireme stages differs in its shape in the genus Aphrophora from that in the other genera as far as studied. It is a much elongated structure early in its appearance in Aphrophora, while it first appears as an oval or round body in the others. All eight species of the Cercopidae studied show a typical odd chromosome, which divides only in the second spermatocyte division. In all of the species except Aphrophora quadrinotata and Clastoptera proteus, in which the material was limited and the equatorial plates consequently not studied in favorable positions, there is one chromosome among the reduced number which is distinctly larger than the others. In no case is the odd chromo- some the largest one. WOODS HOLE, July 30, 1915- THE REACTIONS OF AN ORB-WEAVING SPIDER, EPEIRA SCLOPETARIA CLERCK, TO RHYTHMIC VIBRATIONS OF ITS WEB.1 WILLIAM MORTON BARROWS. WITH THREE PLATES. I. Introduction 316 II. Materials and Methods 316 II. Experiments 3*9 1. Experiments Using Rhythmic Vibrations 319 2. Experiments Using a Y-shaped Vibrator 321 3. Response in the Dark 322 4. Distribution of Vibrations Through the Web 322 5. Mutilation Experiments 323 IV. Discussion and Summary 325 I. INTRODUCTION. The work reported in this paper was suggested by a chance observation2 made in the summer of 1911. A fly was held close to one of the spiders without eliciting any response; when the fly's vibrating wing was allowed to touch a strand of the web, however, the response was instantaneous and positive. The spider ran to the fly and seized it. A vibrating rubber band held against a strand of the web caused a very similar response. During the summer of 1913 these spiders were studied more carefully in an attempt to determine: first whether the stimulus was vibratory in nature or must be considered to be due to some other force and second whether the response could be identified as a "tropism" or taxis. II. MATERIALS AND METHODS. At the Lake Laboratory maintained by the Ohio State Uni- versity at Cedar Point, Ohio, the large orb weaving spider, Epeira sclopetaria, is very abundant, building its webs on the front porch and in the angles of the building and roof. The habit 1 Contribution No. 42 from the Department of Zoology and Entomology, Ohio State University. 2 The note of Boys (80) was not known to the writer until the larger part of these experiments had been carried out. REACTIONS OF ORB-WEAVING SPIDER TO VIBRATIONS OF WEB. 317 of the female of tfcis species of remaining at the center of her web for long periods of time makes it a very convenient form to study in its normal surroundings. This species builds its web in dead branches or in the angles of buildings where there is an abundance of small or medium sized insects. The web usually consists of 17. 18 or 19 relatively inelastic strands which radiate from a center like the spokes of a wheel. These radiating strands are attached at their outer ends to twigs or boards or to guys or stays which anchor several radii to the support. Surrounding the center of the web is an irregular network known as the hub and notched zone which serves as a resting place for the inhabitant of the web. For a short space outside the hub the radii are bare (the free zone) but beyond this is found the viscid spiral consisting of finer strands which are extremely elastic and are beaded with microscopic sticky drops which serve to hold and entangle the insect prey. It is probably the extreme elasticity of these spiral strands which allows them to detain a strong insect without being snapped, thus giving the spider time to reach the detained insect and complete its en- snarement by the addition of fresh silk from the spinneretts. The normal resting position of the female spider is with the head directed downward and the legs spread outward on the notched zone as is shown in Fig. i . The method used to obtain this pho- tograph and the others following is that used by Comstock ('12, p. 181). A female spider was placed on a dead branch held in the neck of a bottle which was set in a tray of water. During the first or second night the spider usually built a perfect web. The branch was then moved to the photographing table with as little disturbance as possible and placed in front of a soap box painted a dull black on the inside. Arranged in this way before the camera it was possible to take pictures showing the spider, web, and vibrator straw, and the various positions taken by the spider in the act of responding to the vibrator. The size of the web varies from two inches in diameter, or even smaller when built by very young spiders, to eighteen inches or more when built by mature females. The male builds a web very much like that made by the female but as he has a roving disposition one is never sure that the same individual can be 3l8 WILLIAM MORTON BARROWS. located twenty-four hours later while the females often live for weeks in the same place, repairing the web every evening but not altering it materially. In crawling across the web the spider always follows a radiating strand or at the edge of the web, one of the guy strands, and places its feet on the radii or on the junctions of the radii and spiral threads where the latter hold no sticky materials. The front feet are usually placed on the same radiating strand but the second and third pairs may be spread out on the two ad- joining strands. It is possible for the spider to crawl rather swiftly along a single strand for a considerable distance, all eight feet using the same thread. In crossing the web the spider usually leaves behind a dragline which may remain across the web, adhering to it after the spider has returned to the center Some individuals on the other hand when they reach the edge of the web swing free, held only by the drag line up which they climb in returning to the center. Occasionally one finds both methods employed by the same individual. Most spiders are not skilful enough to cross the web several times without tearing out or snagging several of the segments of the spiral thread. When the web is violently disturbed the spider usually retreats to a niche or corner (the retreat) and remains there motionless unless again disturbed. Some individuals remain in the retreat instead of at the center of the web. When this is done one forefoot is placed on the trapeline leading to the hub and any activity of the web such as that produced by an entangled insect sends the spider like a flash down to the web. In this connection another fact may be noted; a spider outside the center of the orb always returns to the center, takes the normal position and then orients before it finds an entangled insect. This might be explained as due to the difficulty of crossing the web by any other path than by the radii. However, the inability to orient accurately in any other position than the center gives a clue to a more probable explanation. Individuals of the species Epiera sclopetaria will eat nearly any insects which happen to become entangled in the web. The food of those studied inside the screened porch con- sisted almost entirely of rather large flies of the genera Musca, Sarcophaga, and Lucilia. It is in the snaring of these flies that REACTIONS OF ORB-WEAVING SPIDER TO VIBRATIONS OF WEB. 319 this Epeira seems to be. especially expert. When a fly strikes a web it often goes through, breaking out one or two spiral segments. If, however, it does not break through it hangs for a second, buzz- ing, then breaks one or two of the sticky strands and flies away. A fly seldom entangles itself to such an extent that it cannot get • free inside of five seconds. A successful spider then must reach the fly in less than two or three seconds after it strikes the web. The actual capture of the fly is accomplished usually either by biting the fly and stunning it or by winding it with web. The entangled fly may be left where it struck or may be torn from the web, and carried attached to one of the spider's hind feet to the center of the web where it is thoroughly chewed and its liquid parts swallowed. The apparatus used to produce rhythmic vibrations consisted of three tuning forks and an electric vibrator. One fork had a vibration rate of 100 double vibrations per second, another a rate of about 487 and the third was an adjustable fork with a large range of vibration rates but with very limited amplitude. The electric vibrator was a modified electric door bell in which the clapper was replaced by a long grass straw. The number of vibrations produced by this instrument could be varied to some extent by changing the tension of a spring and a regulator screw, while the amplitude of the vibration varied with the length of straw used. The vibration rate of the vibrator was obtained by comparing a tracing made by it on a sheet of blackened paper on a revolving drum with a simultaneous record made by the tuning fork giving 100 double vibrations per second. The electric vibrator was found to be more effective than a fork because it gave vibrations of equal intensity, i, e., it did not run down. It had also another advantage in that it could be controlled by a switch held in the hand and could be operated at a distance from the operator. A stop watch was used to measure the time elaps- ing between the beginning of the stimulus and the arrival of the spider at the place where the straw touched the web. III. EXPERIMENTS. i. Experiments Using Rhythmic Vibrations. When the vibrator straw is placed against one of the spiral strands or against one of the radii and caused to vibrate the spider 32O WILLIAM MORTON BARROWS. orients instantly and advances along the nearest radius to the straw, seizes the straw with its mandibles and may spread web on the straw with the hind pair of feet (Fig. 3). This reaction is carried out in essentially this manner no matter where the straw may strike the web. The orientation is so rapidly executed and is followed so closely by the forward locomotion that it is difficult to separate the two parts of the response. If, however, the vibrator is set in motion for a fraction of a second only the orienting is accomplished but the forward locomotion toward the vibrator does not follow. A second vibration while the spider is oriented calls forth the forward response and an attack on the vibrator (Fig. 3). The photograph reproduced in Fig. 2 shows such an orientation. If the first vibratory stimulus is not too long or is not followed by a second stimulus the spider usually returns to the resting position at the end of a few seconds. Some individuals, however, follow the orienting response by an interesting series of activities. The fore feet are placed on neighboring radii, drawn toward the animal's body and released suddenly. This release sets the web vibrating parallel to the spider's longitudinal axis. The spider then turns one space to the right or left and repeats the process until she has oriented through a complete circle and set every pair of radii in motion. The use of this activity is seen if there happens to be a captured fly or a piece of dirt in the web. When the two radii which pass on either side of the object are set vibrat- ing the object is also set in motion but its motion is not of the same rate as that of the rest of the web and it sets up an echo or return vibration. To this the spider responds. A dead fly may be rediscovered in this way or a piece of dirt may be located and removed. Responses to different frequencies show considerable variations and it is not possible to predict that a certain individual will respond in a definite way to a given stimulus. This variation in response ranges from instantaneous orientation and forward locomotion to a slow orientation and slow approach toward the vibrating point or it may happen that no sign will be given that the stimulus has been perceived. Roughly speaking a large spider responds most quickly to a vibration of considerable am- REACTIONS OF ORB-WEAVING SPIDER TO VIBRATIONS OF WEB. 321 plitude with a vibration rate of 24 to 300 per second. It was impossible with the materials at hand to construct a vibrator giving a high rate and having also a considerable amplitude, so recourse to steel wires and small forks was necessary. The large spiders did not respond well to wires and forks with high vibra- tion rate and small amplitude but they did respond instantly to the vibrating wings of Chrysops (127 per sec.), Microbembex (208 per sec.), Musca (284 per sec.), where the amplitude ranged from 4 mm. to 10 mm. Small spiders responded quickly to vibrations ranging from 100 per sec. to 487 per sec. and even higher although the amplitude was very small. This difference in responsiveness between the young and old spiders is probably correlated with differences in size and rate of wing vibration of the insects which are ensnared and used as food by young and old. In general small insects have high wing vibration rates while the larger insects have lower rates of wing vibration (Packard, '03. p. 150). The smaller spiders eat small insects and the large spiders eat larger insects. The following species of insects were caught and eaten by E. sclopetaria: Chrysops vitatus (127 vibr. per sec.); Calliphora vomitaria (130 vibr. per sec.) ; Microbembex monodonta (208 vibr. per sec.) ; Musca domes- tica (284 vibr. per sec.). Many small midges (Chironomus and others) were eaten by the young spiders and occasionally by the adults. The vibration rate of these small midges is probably very high, judged by the high pitched note which they give out, but it was impossible at the time to determine its rate. 2. Experiments Using a Y-shaped Vibrator. In order to determine whether the spider reacted to a single vibrating strand or to the center of a vibrating area of the web, a Y-shaped vibrator made up of insulated magnet wire was adjusted to the vibrator and arranged in such a manner that its ends touched the web at two places, 2 or 3 cm. apart. When the vibrator so adjusted was operated the spider responded readily, going to a point on the edge of the web midway between the two vibrating points and then after some slight hesitation going toward one or the other of the vibrator wires (see Fig. 4). If, however, these points of wire were more than 3 cm. apart the spider at the 322 WILLIAM MORTON BARROWS. center of the web usually hesitated, turning first toward one, then toward the other, finally orienting to one and attacking this by itself. 3. Response in the Dark. In order to test the ability to respond in the dark the vibrator was set up late in the afternoon, the straw touching one of the radial strands of a web which was built in the frame of a window. The window was shaded on the outside by a heavy thicket. At 9:30 P.M. the room was so dark that a person standing inside could discern the outline of the window with the utmost difficulty. A flash of light from a pocket electric lamp showed that the female occupying the web was at the center of her web. The vibrator switch was closed and at the end of about four seconds the electric flash light showed the spider biting the vibrator straw in the same manner as that shown in Fig. 3. This experiment indicates that unless these spiders use rays of light which our eyes do not perceive, sight plays no essential part in the orientation to and the ensnaring of the prey. 4. The Distribution of Vibrations through the Web. The distribution of vibrations as they travel across the web is of some theoretical interest. The following method for recording these vibrations was used with considerable success. A spider in its web was placed before the camera and made to respond to the vibrator repeatedly until it would respond no more. A photograph (Fig. 5) of 15 seconds' exposure was then made while the vibrator was in motion. The web was somewhat torn by the spider before it ceased to respond, but the photograph reveals by the thickening of the lines the distribution and amplitude of the vibrations in all parts of the web. The amplitude of the vibrations decreases rapidly from the periphery toward the center. The radial strand connected with the vibrator shows the greatest lateral displacement while the strands on either side of this show less and less disturbance as the distance away from the vibrator increases. A slight thickening of the spiral strands in a direction at right angles to the direction of the primary vi- bration can be noted on the segments directly across the center from the vibrator. The center of the web seems to be the part REACTIONS OF ORB-WEAVING SPIDER TO VIBRATIONS OF WEB. 323 least affected. If there is any motion here it is probably at right angles to the original vibration, that is, it is probably parallel to the spiders' long axis after orientation. 5. Mutilation Experiments. The foregoing experiments coupled with careful observations on the spinning behavior of the orb-weaver lead to the conviction that the organs used in detecting the movements of the web are proably tactile, at least there are no other organs described which would seem to serve the purpose as well. There can be little doubt that sense hairs are very abundant on the legs, par- ticularly on the tarsi of these spiders. These hairs have been described by Dahl (83), Wagner (88), McCook (90), and recently by Mclndoo (n). The functions of these hairs have been inter- preted in various ways, but little or no experimental work has been accomplished other than attempts to show that some spiders hear. Responses to sounds seem to have been observed only in those forms which build webs. It seems likely that responses in the web building forms are due to the vibrations of the air being picked up by the strands of the web (Mclndoo, 'n, p. 412). It was thought desirable to determine if possible the location of the sense-organs used in detecting vibrations. By careful manip- ulation with a pair of fine dissecting scissors it was possible to snip off one or more of a spider's legs without causing the spider to leave the web. It is necessary to use great care not to shake the web because an irregular shaking gives rise to the negative response, the spider running away to the retreat. The contrast between this insensibility to the amputation of legs and extreme sensitiveness to irregular vibrations of the web emphasizes the fact that these spiders receive most if not all of their mechanical stimuli through the web. These operations caused the spider to lose considerable blood but two or three hours usually sufficed to heal the wound. The stumps of the legs were always held up so that they did not touch the web. Experiment i. — After testing a spider to be assured that its responses were normal the two forelegs were cut off as near the middle of the metatarsus as possible. This spider immediately put the stumps of the forelegs into its mouth. The next morning 324 WILLIAM MORTON BARROWS. this spider was in its web. During the night the web had been repaired and a new spiral thread put on. In recording the test made on this spider and those following, IX o'clock, XII o'clock, etc., refers to the position at the edge of the web which corresponds to the same hour on the clock face. Thus VI o'clock is used to designate the edge of the web which the spider normally faces when at rest, i. e., directly downward. Experiment i. — Spider with both forefeet cut off. Fork giving 100 vibrations per second touching web in III o'clock position spider reached fork 8 inches from center in 3 seconds. IX o'clock position reached fork (8 in.) in 2^2 sec. VII o'clock position reached fork (8 in.) in 2^4 sec. Experiment 2. — Spider with third legs cut beween femur and patella. IX o'clock position reacted 8 in. in 2% sec. Ill o'clock position reacted 8 in. in 2>£ sec. XII o'clock position reacted 8 in. in 7^4 sec. This individual showed some difficulty in climbing, but oriented accurately. Experiment 3. — Spider with second legs cut off at patella. Ill o'clock position reacted 7 in. in i^ sec. XII o'clock position reacted 7 in. in i^ sec. X o'clock position reacted 7 in. in i>£ sec. Experiment 4. — Spider with fourth legs cut off at patella. Reactions entirely normal as given above. Another set of experiments which need not be detailed were carried out. In these the right first leg and left fourth leg were cut off and other similar combinations were made. In all cases orientations and the locomotion following were entirely normal except for the slight difficulties in locomotion which might be expected. These experiments indicate that the sense organs used in reacting to the vibratory stimuli are not restricted to any one pair of legs below the metatarsus. There are two possible dis- tributions of sense hairs which would seem to make possible the reactions detailed above ; the sense organs may be confined to the feet, where they come in contact with the web or they may be located on the le.gs or body in such a manner that they pick up REACTIONS OF ORB-WEAVING SPIDER TO VIBRATIONS OF WEB. 325 the vibration of the whole leg or whole body. Hinged sensitive hairs uniformly scattered over the body might answer this pur- pose. It seems most likely, everything considered, that the particular sense organs used are on the tarsi of each leg and come in contact with the web. It is difficult to conceive that an animal whose feet are not extremely sensitive could travel on or manipu- late the delicate strands of these orb-webs. IV. DISCUSSION AND SUMMARY. It is maintained in this paper as in a previous one (Barrows, '07) that an animal exhibits a "tropism" or better a taxis, "when under the influence of [chemical] stimuli acting unilaterally they move toward or away from the source of the stimulus" (Verwrorn, '99, p. 249). It has been shown above that Epeira sclopetaria orients in its web and moves toward the source of a vibratory mechanical stimulus when this is of an appropriate rate and amplitude. Thus this method of response to a vibratory stimulus identifies the reaction as a positive taxis. The term tonotaxis would naturally be used in this connection, but since tonotaxis has been used in another way it seems advisable that the terms positive vibrotaxis should be applied if a short descriptive term is desired. The foregoing may be summarized as follows: 1. Epeira sclopetaria, an orb-weaving spider, starting from the center of its web is able to orient, charge and seize flies which strike and are detained in the web. This process is carried out with extreme rapidity. 2. With the aid of a mechanical vibrator it is possible to show that the stimulus is vibratory, the spider orienting to and attacking the vibrator even in the dark. 3. The response can be analyzed into, (a the orientation, (b) the forward response, and (c) the attack on the vibrating object. The response is in essence a positive vibrotaxis. 4. The vibrations are transmitted through the web in all directions from the vibrating point but the intensity (amplitude) decreases toward the center of the web and on either side. The lines of equal intensity of the vibration form roughly a series of circles the centers of which are at the vibrating point. 326 WILLIAM MORTON BARROWS. 5. The sense organs used in detecting the stimulus are probably sense hairs on the tarsi. 6. This orb-weaving spider provides itself with a temporary extension of its tactile sense organs which makes its tactile sense in reality a distance receptor, much like an auditory or an ol- factory organ. BIBLIOGRAPHY. Barrows, W. M. '07 The Reactions of the Pomace Fly; Drosophila ampelophila Loew, to Odorous Substances. Jour. Exp. Zool., Vol. IV., No. 4, p. 515-53?- Boys, C. V. '80 The influence of a Tuning Fork on the Garden Spider. Nature, Vol. 23 p. 149. Comstock, J. H. '12 The Spider Book. Garden City, N. Y. Dahl, T. '83 Ueber die Horharre bei den Arachniden. Zool. Anz., VI. Davenport, C. B. '08 Experimental Morphology, New York. McCook, H. C. '90 American Spiders and their Spinning Work. Vol. II. Philadelphia. Mclndoo, N. E. 'n The Lyriform Organs and Tactile Hairs of Araneads. Proc. Acad. Nat. Sci. Phil., May, 1911. Packard, A. S. '03 A Text-book of Entomology, New York. Peckham, G. W. and E. G. '87 Some Observations on the Mental Powers of Spiders. Jr. Morph., I., pp 383-4I9- Verworn, M. '99 General Physiology. Trans, by F. S. Lee. London, 8vo. xvi+6ispp. 328 WILLIAM MORTON BARROWS. EXPLANATION OF PLATE I. FIG. i. Showing a female Epeira sclopetaria in the normal resting position in the web. The arrow indicates the place where the vibrator straw touches a radial strand of the web. FIG. 2. The same individual, shown in Fig. i, orienting to the vibrator which had been in motion for a fraction of a second just before the photograph was taken. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXIX FIG. i. W. M. BARROWS. FIG. 2. 330 WILLIAM MORTON BARROWS. EXPLANATION OF PLATE II. FIG. 3. A spider attacking the vibrator straw while it is in motion. FIG. 4. A spider in the act of responding to the Y-shaped vibrator. One prong of the vibrator appears in front, the other behind the spider. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXIX. FIG. 3. W. M. BARROWS. FIG. 4. 332 WILLIAM MORTON BARROWS. EXPLANATION OF PLATE III. FIG. 5. A photograph showing the spider in the normal resting position in the web, while the vibrator is in motion. The arrow indicates the place where the vibrator straw touches a radial strand. The doubling or blurring of the lines of the web shows the distribution of the vibrations. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXIX. W. M. BARROWS. FIG. 5. Vol. XXIX December, 1915. No. 6 BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN OBSERVATIONS ON THE DEVELOPMENT OF COPIDOSOMA GELECHI^E. J. T. PATTERSON. From the Marine Biological Laboratory, and the Zoological Laboratory of the University of Texas (Contribution No. 127). CONTENTS. I. Introduction 333 II. Note on the Life History of Gnorimoschema 336 III. Parasites of Gnorimoschema salinaris 340 IV. Development of Copidosoma gelechiae 341 1. Polygerm Stages 341 (a) Youngest Stages 341 (6) The Nucleated Membrane 343 (c) Precipitated Material 343 (d) The Granular Protoplasm and Embryonic Nuclei 343 (e~) Growth of the Polygerm and Formation of the Primary Divisions or Masses 344 (/) Formation of the Secondary Masses 346 (g) The Inter-embryonal Substance 346 2. Dissociation of the Polygermal Mass 347 3. Pupation, and the Emergence of the Imagines 348 4. The Abortive Embryos 349 V. Number and Sex of Copidosoma Parasites found in Gnorimoschema 352 Summary 358 Literature 359 Plates 362 I. INTRODUCTION. The discovery of polyembryonic development among certain of the hymenopterous parasites has opened up an extremely interesting field for investigation. Like most other important biological discoveries, this one was foreshadowed by the obser- vations of several different naturalists. In a paper of this nature it is not necessary to give an extended account of the history of this discovery. We shall therefore be content with a brief 333 334 J- T- PATTERSON. statement on this point, limiting the account almost entirely to the species with which the paper deals. The general features of polyembryony in insects have been given in the well-known papers of Marchal ('98, '04) and Sil- verstri ('06, '08), but there are many points concerning the details of this process which have not as yet been worked out. It was with the view of studying certain of these details that led the writer three years ago to seek an American species upon which such studies could be made. Dr. L. O. Howard1 suggested that Copidosoma gelechiiz, which parasitizes the larvae of the Solidago gall moth, Gnorimo schema gallcesolidaginis, would be a good form upon which to work, as it seemed to be an undoubted case of polyembryony. The Gnorimoschema moth makes the ellipsoidal galls on the stems of several species of goldenrod. Baron Osten Sacken ('63) seems to have been the first to have published a description of the inflated carcass of the Gnorimoschema larva, caused by the chalcis parasite; but apparently he was not acquainted with the maker of the gall. In 1869 in connection with his account on the life history of this moth, Riley states that the caterpillar serves as a host for no less than six different species of hymenop- terous parasites. One of these, which is shown in his Fig. 6, Plate 2, is described as a "little fly of a dark metallic green color, with reddish legs." This is clearly Copidosoma. Riley states that the larvae of this species infests the caterpillar in great numbers, more than 150 having been obtained from a single host. He supposed that the "mother fly" gnawed a passage through the gall and desposited her batch of eggs in the inmate. He pointed out that the larval parasites cause the caterpillar to swell to three or four times its natural size, and after having absorbed all the juices of the victim, form very small brownish cocoons, which are so packed together that they give to the worm the puffed-up appearance which is typical of the mummified carcasses of lepidopterous larvae that have been parasitized by a polyembryonic species. It was this inflated condition of the larval host that led Riley to call the parasite the " Inflating Chalcis 1 For this as well as for other suggestions received throughout the progress of the work, the writer is greatly indebted to Dr. Howard. DEVELOPMENT OF COPIDOSOMA GELECHI/E. 335 Fly." Howard ('85) later named this species Copidosoma gdechicE. Upon examining the various goldenrods about Woods Hole, Mass., for galls of Gnorimo schema, it was found that Solidago sempervirens furnished the best opportunity for obtaining material. However, the common gall maker of this solidago proved not to be Gnorimoschema gallcesolidaginis Riley, but a closely related species, G. salinaris Busck. The parasites in- festing these two moths are varieties of the same species, Copi- dosoma gelechics. The selection of this species has not proved altogether satis- factory, because the gall-making habit of the host complicates the life history and renders the collecting of material for early stages of the parasite somewhat more difficult than from a host which feeds openly. Furthermore, the moth, and likewise the parasite, has but one generation a year. In addition to these objections, there is the further one that the egg of Copidosoma gives rise to a relatively large number of individuals (about 191 on the average). In attempting to obtain material for the studies which the writer has in mind, it seems best to seek to find a host which is an open or semi-open feeder, which has two or more generations a year, and which harbors a parasitic egg giving rise to but few individuals. During the past summer at least two species have been found which in the main seem to fulfill these conditions. It therefore seems best to publish the main facts concerning the development of Copidosoma before giving it up for more favorable material. There is one feature in the development of Copidosoma which makes further study desirable. We refer to the abortive em- bryos (presently to be described), which at first were thought to be comparable to the so-called asexual larvse of Litomastix truncatdlus. It will be recalled that Silvestri ('06) described in this species the development of both sexual and asexual larvae from a single egg. In one instance he secured from a caterpillar of Plusia gamma 1,700 sexual and 220 asexual larvae of Litomastix. He believes that the asexual larvae play the role of raspers for the normal larvae, tearing the tissues of the host so that the sexual larvae may the more easily secure the necessary food. It may 336 J. T. PATTERSON. be stated here that the abortive larvae of Copidosoma are in no way comparable to the asexual larvae of Litomastix as described by Silvestri. II. NOTE ON THE LIFE HISTORY OF GNORIMOSCHEMA. In order to collect polyembryonic material it is essential to know something about the life history of the host; especially is this true in cases like Gnorimo schema in which the larval host is a gall maker. Considerable attention has therefore been given to a study of the life history of G. salinaris. The general habits of the Solidago gall moths were first made known by Riley's ('69) studies on G. gallcBSolidaginis . According to Riley this species winters over in the imago stage and may be seen flying in the month of May. When the young plants (Solidago nemoralis) are about six inches high the female moth lays her egg either in the terminal bud or at the side of the stalk immediately below the bud. The young caterpillar upon hatch- ing burrows into the stalk and starts the development of the gall. By the first of June the gall has just begun to form and contains a larva about one-third grown. The larva and its ellipsoidal gall reach their full size by the middle of July. The caterpillar which now measures over half an inch in length prepares for the change into the chrysalis state by first eating a round passage- way through the wall well toward the upper end of the gall. The orifice is then closed by a secretion of liquid silk, which hardens to form a silken plug. After closing the orifice, the caterpillar lines the passage-way and the walls with a delicate silk, and then transforms into a shiny, mahogany-brown pupa, about a half inch long. The moths begin to emerge about the middle of August and continue to appear until the beginning of October. Many phases of the life history of G. salinaris are similar to those of G. gallaesolidaginis , but there are some important differ- ences. The earliest date at which galls of the marsh goldenrod have been secured was June 12, 1914, and at that time many of the galls were well started. Between June 12 and 15, 63 galls of various sizes were collected and examined. They varied in size from 8 to 12 mm. in length and from 4 to 17 mm. in transverse section. In shape the galls also vary greatly. Some are distinctly DEVELOPMENT OF COPIDOSOMA GELECHI/E. 337 pear-shaped, while others are fusiform, with various gradations between these two general types. The galls occur at different heights on the stem, but the vast majority of them are located at or near the base of the stalk (Fig. i). Their position is un- doubtedly determined by the location of the point at which the larva penetrates the young shoot. If this point is located toward the base of the young stalk, the gall will naturally appear near the base of the fully grown plant; but if it is located in or near the terminal bud, the gall will appear some little distance up on the stem. Occasionally two galls are found on the same plant (Fig. 8). A few cases have been observed in which the gall was located at the tip of the terminal bud, producing a stunted plant without a central, flower-bearing stalk. With these few exceptions, the gall of G. salinaris does not seem materially to affect the growth and vigor of the plant. It is true that many galls are found on plants that are apparently stunted but such dwarfing is to be attributed to the adverse conditions under which the plant sometimes grows. In regions that are very much exposed to the wind, like the banks along the coast, many of the goldenrods are small and clearly dwarfed; but this condition applies as well to the plants that are free from galls as to those that are infected. The habits of gall making are similar in the three common species of Gnorimoschema, although the following differences may be pointed out. G. gallceasteriella produces a triangular gall at the top of the dwarfed or stunted stems of Solidago ccesia, S. axil- laris, S. latifolis, and Aster divaricatus -1 The form of the gall differs somewhat with the plant. The gall of G. gallaesolidaginis may occur toward the top of the stem, but usually it is located just below the middle, especially is this true of the galls on S. can- adensis. The galls of this moth do not dwarf the plant. The condition of the galls of G. salinaris on the marsh goldenrod has already been described. They occur nearer the base of the stem than do those of last species, and like the latter there is little or no tendency to dwarfing the plant. The larvae secured from the galls collected between June 12 and 15 varied from 3 to 8 mm. in length. Beginning with the middle of June, the young caterpillars grow rapidly, doubling 1 Part of these data were kindly furnished the writer by Dr. T. M. Forbes. 338 J. T. PATTERSON. their size within a fortnight. By the middle of July they have reached their full growth, and are beginning to show signs of undergoing pupation, which is evidenced by the construction of the passage-way. The passage-way and its orifice differ in two respects from those of G. gallcesolidaginis as described by Riley ('69). The silk lining does not extend much beyond the lower limits of the passage-way, and hence does not cover the inner surface of the wall. The second difference is seen in the character of the orifice and its silk plug. The caterpillar of G. salinaris does not cut the passage-way quite through the wall, but leaves the very thin epidermis of the stem, which is used as a back- ground for the construction of the plug (Fig. 7). TABLE I. TABLE SHOWING DATES OF PUPATION AND EMERGENCE OF COPIDOSOMA AND GNORIMOSCHEMA. Pupation (Beginning of) Emergence • Copidosoma Gnorimoschema Copidosoma Gnorimoschema Aug. 6, IQI2. Aug. 5. 1913. July 3i. 1914. .July 30, I9I5- Aug. 6, 1912. July 23. 1913. July 30, 1914. .July 26, I9I5- Aug. 25 to Sept. 12, 1912. Sept 3 to Sept. 13. I9I3- Aug. 30 to Sept. 18, 1914. .Aug. 24 to Sept. 21, I9I5- Aug. 25 to Sept. IO, 1912. Aug. 25 to Sept. 10, I9I3- Aug. 22 to Sept. II, 1914. Aug. 24 to Sept. 14- 1915. Pupation occurs during the last week of July and the first week of August (Table I.). The imagines begin to emerge about August 25, and continue to appear until September 10. The moth has been seen flying in the open during this period. Females kept in captivity often lay eggs. This they do within ten days after emerging, and irrespective of their association with males. As a rule the moths simply drop the eggs on the bottom of the cage, or they may lay them on the leaves and flowers of goldenrods placed in the cage. At first it was thought that G. salinaris must differ from G. gattcBsolidaginis in respect to its egg-laying habits, for Riley states that the latter species although emerging in the fall, hibernates as an imago and lays ^H DEVELOPMENT OF COPIDOSOMA GELECHI^E. 339 its eggs in the following May. It has been discovered, however, that G. gallasolidaginis from the galls of S. canadensis in western Ohio likewise drops several eggs soon after emerging from the pupa in September. This raises the question as to whether these fall eggs develop into larvae, for if so it would be difficult to explain how the young caterpillars could withstand the winter and succeed in the spring in finding a young goldenrod bud or shoot in which to start the new gall. In reply to an inquiry, Mr. A. Busck of Washington kindly informed the writer that the laying, of eggs by Gnorimoschema was of no particular significance, as it is not uncommon for certain Lepidoptera to drop their eggs prematurely, especially if kept in captivity. In view of this fact an observation made in the fall of 1913 is of special interest. During the first few days of Sep- tember of that year a single female, confined in a cage with several males, laid a dozen or more eggs on goldenrod leaves and flowers. On the thirteenth of the month three larvae hatched from this batch of eggs! There can be no possible doubt as to the correct- ness of this observation, for the hatching of one of the little cater- pillars was actually observed under a hand lens. It is difficult to explain the development of these larvae from fall eggs, except on the basis of parthenogenesis. It is true that the female which laid the eggs from which the larvae developed had been confined with males; but although males and females have been kept together for several weeks during each of the last three seasons, yet mating has never been observed. The sup- position that the fall eggs of G. salinaris may develop by parthen- ogenesis receives strong support from a study of sections of eggs laid by a female not associated with males. In Fig. 20 is shown a transverse section of one of her eggs and it can clearly be seen that development is well started. Twelve eggs out of the batch were sectioned, and it was found that eleven had started to develop, although apparently not in a normal manner. It is not improbable that some few eggs may develop normally and eventually produce larvae. The question of parthenogenesis in the Solidago moths is one needing further study. It might be worth while to add that parthenogenetic develop- ment among Lepidoptera is by no means unknown. DeGeer is 34° J- T. PATTERSON. given credit for having first discovered long ago that certain butterflies belonging to the genus Solenobia lay unfertilized eggs which develop into normal imagines, and later von Siebold not only confirmed this observation, but also discovered that Psyche helix reproduced parthenogenetically. It has since been shown by several workers that the silk moth, Bombyx mori, may under certain conditions reproduce by parthenogenesis. III. PARASITES OF GNORIMOSCHEMA SALINARIS. Riley reports six hymenopterous parasites for Gnorimo schema gallcesolidaginis , and in addition to these he found a beetle larva and another lepidopterous larva which intrude as inquilines within the cavity of the gall made by this species. At least five hymenopterous parasites have been found associated with G. salinaris. The most important of these is Copidosoma gelechice, which is by far the most common parasite attacking the moth. The other four species are CalUephialtes notanda Cress, Epiurus sp., Eurytoma sp. (pupa), and Pseudacrias sex- dentatus Girault. The first of these four occurs most frequently, while the last has been observed but a few times. However, it is of special interest, inasmuch as it is the only observed case of a second parasite emerging along with Copidosoma, although the larvae of other species have been found associated with the larvae of Copidosoma. On September 3, 1914, six individuals, all females, emerged together with a brood of about one hundred Copidosomas from a single carcass. The small pupae of Pseu- dacrias lying among those of Copidosoma were observed through the transparent chitin of the carcass of the host some days prior to their emergence. They were not grouped together but scat- tered about in different parts of the carcass. Each pupa was inclosed in a chamber very much smaller than, but exactly similar to that containing a Copidosoma pupa. Usually Pseudacrias larvae do not pupate until after the larval host has undergone this process. About a dozen Gnorimoschema pupae have been found containing Pseudacrias pupae, which later emerged. It is not probable that Pseudacrias is polyembryonic. First, because both male and female individuals usually emerge from the same pupal host; and second, because the individuals DEVELOPMENT OF COPIDOSOMA GELECHI/E. 34! do not come out at the same time. The single instance of six females ssuing simultaneously with the brood of Copidosoma can be explained by assuming that a single female deposited six fertilized eggs in the host at the same time. However, this case is of special interest as it demonstrates the synchronous develop- ment in a single host of the broods of two distinct parasites, and thus supports Wheeler's ('10) suggested explanation of Silvestri's so-called asexual larvae in Litomastix. In addition to the five hymenopterous parasites, there are two insect larvae associated with the larva of G. salinaris. They are undoubtedly inquilines. One of these is a beetle and the other a lepidopterous larva (Fig. 5). Judging from Riley's account, these two species are very similar to if not identical with the cor- responding inquilines reported by him for the galls of G. galltz- solidaginis. IV. DEVELOPMENT OF COPIDOSOMA GELECHI^:. i. The Poly germ Stages. (a) Youngest Stages. — We have not secured the cleavage stages of Copidosoma, owing to the fact that they occur earlier in the year than we have been able to reach Woods Hole. There- fore, in describing the developmental processes which have their inception in the cleavage stages, we must rely upon the work of other investigators in this field for our interpretation of the sig- nificance of these processes. The youngest stages secured were found in a small larva of Gnorimo schema, taken June 21, 1913. The series of sections of this small caterpillar contains three young polygerms of Copi- dosoma. Evidently the egg from which the caterpillar developed had had three parasitic eggs deposited in it. Two of the poly- germs, which lie close together, are situated in the first and second body segments of the larva, just beneath the hypodermis; while the third is found in sections 5 to 14 posterior to these, and is also situated just beneath the hypodermis of the host. The three polygerms are not of the same size, as is indicated by the following measurements : Of the two specimens lying close together, the larger measures 150^1 by 82 /JL and runs through 15 sections (150 /*), the smaller measures 103 n by 71 fj., and is 342 J- T. PATTERSON. found in 12 sections; the single specimen measures 179 /x by 95 /JL and occupies 8 sections only. In structure the three polygerms are practically identical. Each consists of two distinct zones: (i) An outer relatively thick zone containing a large number of nuclei irregularly placed, and (2) a central region containing the embryonic nuclei (Fig. 19). In the absence of the earlier stages, it is not an easy matter to interpret the conditions seen in these polygerms. In the main they correspond most nearly to the conditions in the egg of Litomastix (Copidosoma} truncatellus , as described by Silvestri ('06). I therefore interpret the outer zone to be the product of the "polar ooplasm" plus the "polar nuclei," while the central region contains the true embryonic nuclei, derived from the fertilized nucleus, or in the case of parthenogenetic development, from the matured egg nucleus. There is of course one essential difference in the corresponding stages of Litomastix and Copidosoma. In the polygerm of the former the central region is composed of a solid mass consisting of distinct cells, while in the latter this region is on the point of being broken into multi-nucleated masses, which form the pri- mordia of the embryos (cf. Fig. 19 A with Silvestri's Fig. 33, PI. III.). It may be that the embryonic nuclei are delimited by cell walls in Copidosoma, although one can not make them out with certainty, even under the highest powers of the micro- scope. Judging from the work of other investigators, one would expect to find the embryonic nuclei surrounded by cell walls. In Ageniaspis, Marchal ('04) first reported that the early embry- onal masses were pluri-nuclear in character, but Silvestri ('08) and Martin ('14) have later demonstrated that the nuclei are surrounded by cell walls. In Copidosoma the embryonic nuclei are often so closely packed together that the demonstration of cell walls would be extremely difficult. The three polygerms mentioned above are of particular interest, in that they show very clearly the manner in which the central mass with its nuclei breaks up to form the primordia of the multiple embryos. The central region itself consists of two distinct substances, (i) A granular protoplasm in which the embryonic nuclei lie, and (2) a more fluid-like material which DEVELOPMENT OF COPIDOSOMA GELECHI/E. 343 becomes greatly shrunken during the process of fixation, and which in sections appears as a precipitated substance (Fig. 19 A, P.M.}. As to the origin of these different substances we know nothing, but their subsequent history is clear. For the sake of clearness in description we shall use the following terms: (i) l$u- deated Membrane for the outer zone; (2) Granular Layer for the protoplasm containing the embryonic nuclei ; and (3) Precipitated Material for the shrunken fluid-like substance. (b) The Nucleated Membrane. — In these young polygerms the outer zone stains more deeply than the central mass. The "polar nuclei" have no definite arrangement, but are irregularly scattered throughout the protoplasm. The entire zone therefore is in every sense of the word a syncytium. As the polygerm grows in size the nuclei become arranged into a single layer, and the protoplasm thins out, thus forming a true nucleated membrane about the central or embryonic portion of the egg (Fig. 21, N.M.). In the later history of the polygerm some of the nuclei are clearly surrounded by cell walls, that is, there is a tendency for the membrane to become cellular. At first the young polygerms are naked, that is there are no elements from the host tissue laid down on the outer surface of the nucleated membrane. Later a few mesenchyme cells are found on the surface of the membrane, and still later these cells give rise to the adipose tissue (Fig. 22, A.T.}, which may com- pletely surround the polygerm. (c) Precipitated Material. — This material occupies the central portion of the polygerm. Apparently it is formed through the action of the fixing reagent upon the fluid-like protoplasm. In sections it is very much shrunken, thus leaving an irregular clear space (Fig. 21, C). As we shall see later, it persists throughout the entire polygerm phase of development. (d) The Granular Protoplasm and the Embryonic Nuclei. — In Fig. 19 the condition of the embryonic nuclei and their surround- ing granular protoplasm is especially clear. Most of the nuclei are indifferently scattered in the protoplasm, but some of them are collecting into groups. The number of nuclei in each group is variable; some groups contain only two or three nuclei, while others may have as many as ten or twelve. The granular pro- 344 J- T. PATTERSON. toplasm surrounding a group of nuclei soon rounds off and the primordial embryo with its surrounding layer lies free within the more fluid contents of the central region of the egg (Fig. 19 A}. The more usual condition is for the spherical mass to remain attached at one side to the peripheral layer of the granular pro- toplasm (Fig. 19 B, P.E.). Eventually all of the embryonic nuclei thus become included in these spherical masses of pro- toplasm, and thus become isolated as primordia of the embryos. The condition at the close of the formation of the primordia is shown in Fig. 21. This specimen was found in a series of sections of a 3 mm. caterpillar, taken June 15, 1914. In the median section it measures 113 M by 203 /i, and runs through 40 sections (280^1). It lies in the middle portion of the body cavity, to one side of the intestine, which on account of the size of the polygerm is pushed out of place. As compared with the preceding polygerms this one is very much larger and shows a number of important changes. The nucleated membrane has become much thinner and its nuclei are arranged more or less into a single layer. The adipose tissue is being laid down on the outer surface of the membrane. The most important change, however, has occurred in the embryonic masses themselves. The protoplasm which surrounds a group of nuclei is differenti- ated into two distinct regions. The central part, crowded with nuclei, stains somewhat lighter than the peripheral zone, which forms a relatively dense layer about the central core (Fig. 21, P.E.). There are still a few nuclei which have not as yet been surrounded by the dense layer, but this stage marks approxi- mately the end of the division of the germ into separate embryos. (e) Growth of the Polygerm and the Formation of the Primary Divisions or Masses. — Upon the completion of the primitive embryos, the polygerm grows very rapidly. It first extends in the direction of its long axis, soon transforming into an elongated cylindrical structure. One specimen showing this condition measures in section 148 /j. by 430 yu. It never becomes an elon- gated tube as does the polygerm of Ageniaspis. During this growth the adipose tissue is laid down in the form of a thick layer about the polygerm. One of the easiest ways in which to find a polygerm of this and later stages is to examine the large DEVELOPMENT OF COPIDOSOMA GELECHI/E. 345 fat bodies lying in the middle region of the body cavity of the larval host. If the caterpillar is parasitized one of these bodies is almost certain to contain the polygerm. After the elongated condition is attained, the further growth of the polygerm may take place in any direction. In some cases the extension is mainly in one plane, thus transforming the polygerm into a flat, plate-like structure (Fig. 13). In other cases it forms a thick irregular mass (Fig. u), and when viewed as a whole mount shows many elevations on its surface, due to the breaking up of the entire polygerm into single masses, each of which contains an embryo. During the rapid expansion of the polygerm a very important change takes place in its structure, whereby each embryo become enclosed in a double involucre. The first step in this process begins just prior to that period of development in which the polygerm attains its elongated, cylindrical shape. It consists in the formation of constrictions in the nucleated membrane which break up the single polygerm into a series of primary divisions or masses (Fig. 15). In the specimen shown in this figure there are about twelve of these masses. Each primary mass has the same general structure as the original single polygerm. It is surrounded by a portion of the nucleated membrane, con- tains precipitated material, and has a variable number of em- bryos, from five to fifteen or more. In Fig. 22 one end of a longitudinal section of a polygerm is shown with the completed primary masses. Three of these masses are seen in the figure, together with a portion of a fourth. Attention should be called to the fact that the adipose tissue, although in contact with the polygerm, is still a distinct structure. In the process of forming the primary masses not all of the elements of the nucleated membrane are taken into these structures. Some of them are left behind and later lie in the inter-embryonal spaces or interstices. In Fig. 22 a number of these elements (cells and nuclei) are shown at the point marked " N," lying between the primary masses and the adipose layer. In another portion of the same polygerm a single primary mass is being constricted off laterally. It appears as a bud ex- tending from the main body of the polygerm. It is such cases 346 J. T. PATTERSON. as this which give rise to the condition frequently seen in whole mounts, in which the surface of the polygerm displays many protuberances. (/) Formation of the Secondary Masses. — The primary masses soon become broken up into secondary masses. This is also brought about by constrictions of the nucleated membrane (Fig. 23). These secondary masses may contain more than one embryo, in which case they immediately form constrictions which result in producing still smaller masses, each of which contains a single embryo. In the constrictions which lead to the cutting off of a single embryo with its involucres, some of the precipitated material is enclosed between that portion of the granular layer which is in contact with the embryo and that part lying adjacent to the inner surface of the nucleated membrane. These two parts of the granular layer then fuse, forming a single involucre in which are the spaces containing the precipitated material (Fig. 24). The embryo is thus surrounded by two involucres, a granular layer, and a nucleated membrane (Fig. 26). In some cases the precipitated material may be so abundant as to form a solid zone between the inner and outer parts of the granular layer; in others it is small in amount and gives the appearance of much flattened nuclei lying within this layer (Fig. 26, P.M.}. (g) The Inter-embryonal Substance. — At the close of the for- mation of the single embryonic masses and their involucres the inter-embryonal interstices are already filled with a substance derived from several different sources. It consists of a plasma- like matrix in which are embedded cells and nuclei. We have already noted that during the formation of the primary and secondary masses some of the elements from the nucleated membrane are not included in the outer involucre, but are left in the inter-embryonal spaces. During the early history of the inter-embryonal substance, it consists mainly of product from this membrane. Later cells from two other sources enter into its formation. First, leucocytes from the host are found embedded in the matrix. They are especially abundant in those regions of the polygerm exposed directly to the body cavity, that is near a surface barren of adipose tissue. Second, fat cells DEVELOPMENT OF COPIDOSOMA GELECHI^. 347 from the adipose layer invade the inter-embryonal spaces. The fat cells are the last elements to enter the inter-embryonal sub- stance. In Fig. 13 a wedge-shaped mass of fat tissue is seen lying between the embryos in the middle region of the polygerm, on the upper side. Perhaps it would be more correct to say that the embryos bud out into the adipose tissue. Thus in Fig. 24 a single primary mass has been budded off into the adipose tissue. The final condition of the polygerm at the end of the formation of the inter-embryonal substance is shown in Fig. 16. The adipose tissue has invaded the inter-embryonal substance from all sides of the polygerm and has become an organic part of this substance. The fat body and the included polygerm become an extremely complex structure, which may be called the poly- germal mass. 2. Dissociation of the Polygermal Mass. The setting free of the larval parasites into the body cavity of the host is brought about through the dissociation or disin- tegration of the inter-embryonal substance. The fat brought into close contact with the embryos by the invasion of the adipose tissue is digested and absorbed by them. It is therefore the first component of the inter-embryonal substance to disappear. That the fat is digested and consumed by the embryos is evi- denced by the fact that the numerous other fat bodies remain intact during this period. The disappearance of the fat leaves the embryos loosely held together by the plasmalike matrix, which in turn soon disintegrates, freeing the larvae. The first larvae to be set free are those situated at the periphery of the polygermal mass. Such larvae are usually the largest present in the mass. As the inter-embryonal substance slowly disintegrates the remainder of the larvae are gradually set free (Fig. 17). The earliest date at which free larvae have been found was July 19; the latest, July 31. In the vast majority of cases the mass dissociates during the last week of July. The larvae retain the involucres for some time after being set free (Fig. 18). Once free in the body cavity they proceed to devour the contents of the host, first consuming the fat tissue, and finally the various organs. The last internal organ to dis- appear is the intestine. 34-8 J- T. PATTERSON. 3. Pupation, and the Emergence of the Imagines. Pupation in Copidosoma occurs during the first ten days of August. The pupa stage lasts twenty-eight days. As stated above, the larvae destroy all of the internal organs of the host, and consume such portions as are dissolved by the action of their salivary secretions. The undissolved portion consists largely of the chitinous parts of the trachese. The larvae also destroy all of the body wall except the superficial layer of chitin. During the process of pupation the non-digested content of the cater- pillar hardens and forms the thin-walled, oval chambers in which the parasitic larvae lie and in which they undergo their trans- formation into pupae. The superficial layer is perfectly trans- parent, and at first is very flexible. Later, as drying occurs, it shrinks in on the walls of chambers and becomes hard and rigid, the whole forming the typical mummified carcass (Figs. 2, 4, 6). Practically all of the pupae are oriented in a definite fashion in the carcass. Their heads are directed toward the anterior end of the carcass. Just before becoming immobile, the Gnorimo- schema larva almost invariably turns the head upward in the gall chamber; likewise, the parasitic larvae, just before pupating, orient themselves so that their heads are directed upward, in the direction of the anterior end of the carcass. The imagines come out during the last week of August and the first week of September (Table I.). They escape by gnawing holes through the walls of the chambers and the superficial chitinous layer, both of which become very fragile. As a rule they all emerge practically at the same time. Several cases have been observed in which the entire brood has escaped within a period of ten minutes. Once free from the carcass, they immediately gnaw a hole through the wall of the gall. Their escape is greatly facilitated by the habit of the caterpillar, just before becoming immobile, of eating out a passage-way to, or nearly to the epidermis of the plant. But in no case does the parasitized caterpillar secrete a silken plug. Hence, in order to escape to the exterior, the parasites have only to cut through the remaining thin portion of the wall. The parasites must winter over in the imago state; otherwise DEVELOPMENT OF COPIDOSOMA GELECHI^E. 349 they would not be able to parasitize the normal or spring eggs of Gnorimo schema. Copulation, however, takes place immediately after the adults emerge, but the females do not parasitize the TABLE II. TABLE SHOWING VARIATION IN LENGTH OF LARVAE IN THREE LOTS OF COPIDOSOMA. Length in Lines. Lots. Length in Lines. Lots. I II III I II III I 29 3 3 2 30 10 7 3 2 31 3 i 4 3 32 I 5 5 10 33 i i 6 3 14 34 2 3 7 13 35 4 3 8 I 12 36 2 I 9 4 37 8 5 10 I 3 38 I 4 n i 4 39 I 2 2 12 2 40 3 3 9 13 2 4i 4 4 4 14 2 42 2 7 5 15 I 6 43 2 3 7 16 I 3 44 I 3 7 17 4 4 12 45 3 5 2 18 i 7 46 I 8 4 19 i 6 16 47 I 5 3 20 4 17 48 I 10 i 21 9 49 2 13 2 22 9 7 50 10 2 23 5 6 5i 4 24 3 4 52 2 8 25 3 13 53 i 26 3 5 54 27 I 4 55 I 2 28 4 I 56 fall eggs of this moth. Only on one occasion has an attempt to ovipost in such eggs been observed. In this instance the few females which made the attempt were not able to penetrate the shell of the egg with the ovipositer. 4. The Abortive Embryos. One of the most interesting discoveries made in connection with the study of Copidosoma is what we shall call the abortive embryos or larvae, to which brief reference has already been made. Abortive embryos occur in the development of many different species of both invertebrates and vertebrates. They 35O J. T. PATTERSON. are especially common in mammals. For example, my colleague, Dr. C. G. Hartman, has found a great mortality of embryos in the development of the opossum. Degenerating embryos are found throughout the brief but entire period of gestation. Abortive embryos have been found in at least three other species which have a polyembryonic type of development. One of the two embryos which develop from a single egg of the earthworm, Lumbricus trapezoides, sometimes degenerates. Fernandez ('09) has observed rudimentary embryos in the South American armadillo, Tatusia hybrida, and I have on several occasions seen them in the blastocyst of Tatusia novemcincta. But in no case with which we are acquainted is their number and constancy of occurrence so striking as in Copidosoma. Our attention was first attracted to these abortive embryos while dissecting out a batch of larvae from a large caterpillar. Most of the larvae in the lot were large and about on the point of undergoing pupation, but in addition to these large individuals, there were a number of smaller ones. At first it was supposed that two distinct species of parasitic larvae were present, or that we had a condition similar to that described by Silvestri for Litomastix, of sexual and asexual larvae. It was noted, however, that the small larvae had the same general structure as the larger individuals, except that they still possessed the two involucres typical of all of the younger larvae of this species. A study of serial sections of more than a hundred polygerms has completely demonstrated beyond any possibility of doubt that the small rudimentary embryos are derived from the same egg as larger normal larvae, and consequently do not belong to a different species. The sections show that degenerating embryos are to be found in every stage of development of the polygerm, from the time of the formation of single embryos until the larvae are set free into the body cavity of the host. In Fig. 24 is shown a degenerating embryo which has not yet been completely cut off from its fellow by the constriction of the nucleated membrane. Its nuclei have already completely disintegrated. In Fig. 26 is another embryo well on the way to complete disintegration. Finally Fig. 17, which is a portion of a polygermal mass about at the close of dissociation, contains at least four or five rudimentary embryos. They stain darker than the normal individuals. DEVELOPMENT OF COPIDOSOMA GELECHI^E. 351 The degeneration of embryos or larvae does not cease immedi- ately after the dissociation of the polygermal mass, but such embryos are found up until the beginning of pupation. About fifty lots of free larvae have been dissected out of caterpillars, and TABLE III. TABLE SHOWING THE NUMBER OF PARASITES IN FEMALE BROODS. Brood. No. of Individuals. Brood. No, of Individuals. I 25 46 20O 2 42 47 2OI 3 49 48 2O7 4 52 49 2IO 5 73 50 210 6 89 Si 212 7 91 52 213 8 95 53 213 9 IOO 54 214 10 IOO 55 215 ii 1 06 56 215 12 108 57 216 13 H5 58 216 14 119 59 217 IS I2O 60 229 16 121 61 234 I? 122 62 236 18 124 63 236 19 124 64 237 20 125 65 245 21 131 66 248 22 137 67 250 23 142 68 251 24 145 69 254 25 146 70 256 26 ISO 7i 257 27 151 72 260 28 153 73 261 29 154 74 264 30 156 75 272 31 161 76 275 32 163 77 280 33 164 78 284 34 167 79 286 35 174 80 292 36 174 Si 301 37 178 82 314 38 i?8 83 328 39 179 84 335 40 181 85 338 4i 183 86 340 42 189 87 347 43 192 88 378 44 194 89 385 45 195 90 395 Total = 17,864. Average = 198.48. 352 J. T. PATTERSON. » almost without exception degenerating individuals were found. During the early period of the free larval stage, any given lot will show great variation in the size of the larvae. To show this, all of the individuals of three lots have been measured in the terms of lines on the eye-piece micrometer scale (Table II.). In Lot I. there were only thirty-two larvae. All but six of these would have reached maturity. Lot II. contained 176 larvae, but at least twenty of these were degenerating. Lot III. contained 257 larvae, and probably more than a hundred of them would have degenerated eventually. A series of sketches of these larvae is shown in Fig. 25, A to H. The first four or five of these types would have developed to maturity, but such larvae as those illustrated in F to H degenerate. The most common types of degenerating embryos are the small spherical or oval-shaped masses (G, H). In one extreme case the lot of embryos consisted of about thirty of these masses, together with only a single normal larva. Doubtless many other similar masses had already degenerated. It is difficult to assign any definite cause to the degeneration of these embryos, although it probably has something to do with nutrition. In some cases it seems to be due to the fact that the division of the egg has been carried too far. Some of the pri- mordia receive but few embryonic nuclei, and these are invariably the first to degenerate in the polygerm. In other cases the degeneration is apparently due to the lack of proper nutrition. Most of the polygerms are early surrounded by the thick layer of adipose tissue, upon which the early development of the embryos depends. But other polygerms are almost if not entirely barren of adipose cells, and it is an observed fact that the mor- tality of embryos in such cases is exceedingly high. In Fig. 14 one of these cases is shown. This polygerm, which is devoid of fat tissue, contains more than a hundred embryos, not more than thirty or thirty-five of which have developed normally. V. NUMBER AND SEX OF COPIDOSOMA PARASITES FOUND IN GNORIMOSCHEMA. The number of matured parasites developing in the Gnorimo- schema larva has been determined in 162 cases. This has been DEVELOPMENT OF COPIDOSOMA GELECHLE. 353 done by removing the carcass from the gall chamber a short time before the emergence of the parasites, and enclosing it in a small vial. After all of the parasites have emerged they are killed by filling the vial with 80 per cent, alcohol, and then counted under a binocular microscope. This procedure has the advantage of eliminating the possibility of contamination from other polyembryonic broods. Furthermore, the use of the bi- nocular in counting enables one to distinguish readily the two sexes. The strong sexual dimorphism in Copidosoma makes this task rather easy. The females have the enlarged club-shaped, TABLE IV. TABLE SHOWING THE NUMBER OF PARASITES IN MALE BROODS. Brood. No. of Individuals. Brood. No. of Individuals. I 41 32 178 2 53 33 179 3 6l 34 180 4 67 35 180 5 90 36 180 6 93 37 182 7 96 38 190 8 IOO 39 190 9 101 40 192 10 106 4i 199 it 107 42 199 12 H3 43 202 13 118 44 2O4 14 119 45 214 15 124 46 215 16 124 47 218 17 124 48 223 18 127 49 225 19 128 50 232 20 136 5i 233 21 137 52 236 22 138 53 236 23 139 54 245 24 142 55 247 25 147 56 272 26 152 57 277 2? 168 58 278 28 171 59 323 2Q 172 60 324 30 177 61 328 31 177 62 345 Total = 19,874. Average = 175.32. terminal segment of the antenna, and bright yellow legs, while the males do not have the enlarged segment and the legs are of a 354 J- T- PATTERSON. dark, more or less mottled color. One can therefore readily detect a mixed brood under the microscope. The 162 broods studied were taken at random from the field, and therefore in all probability the data on numbers and sex yielded by them represent the approximate sex ratio for the species. These 162 broods contained a total of 31 ,001 individuals, or an average of over 191 to the brood. Ninety of these, or 55.56 per cent., contained only female parasites, 62, or 38.27 per cent., contained only male parasites, and 10, or 6.17 per cent., con- tained mixed broods of males and females. There are therefore not only a larger number of female broods than male, but the average number of individuals in the former exceed that of the latter. Female broods average a little over 198 individuals to the brood (Table III.), while male broods average only about 175 (Table IV.). The range in the number of individuals in these broods (from 25 to 395 in the female, and from 41 to 345 in the male) makes it evident that these averages are of little significance, except, perhaps, to show that the fertilized egg is slightly more prolific than the unfertilized egg. Of the total number of individuals (31,001), 63.41 per cent, are females and 36.59 per cent, males; but obviously the true sex ratio can not be based on these figures. It must be determined from the number of male and female broods. It would not be a difficult matter to determine this ratio were it not for the un- certainty of the origin of some broods. There is always the pos- sibility in these insects that more than one parasitic egg has been laid in the egg of the host, and hence the parasites which later emerge may not constitute a true polyembryonic brood, but in fact represent two or even more such broods. Under the circumstances, the best that one can do is to determine approxi- mately the sex ratio for the species. This can be done in the following manner. If we assume, as all previous workers have done, that each of the mixed broods is the product of at least two eggs, then, in accordance with the law of probability, we can determine the number of unmixed male and female broods, each of which must also have been produced from two eggs. Worked out on this basis, it is found that the ratio of females to males is 106/76 or a sex ratio of approximately 3 : 2 DEVELOPMENT OF COPIDOSOMA GELECHLE. 355 This leads to a discussion of mixed broods, and especially to a consideration of the question as to how such broods have come into existence. The obvious explanation of their origin is the one given above, viz., that they arise from two eggs. Marchal and Silvestri, who have studied the development of polyembry- onic insects, both offer this explanation. They support the con- clusion by citing the fact that two (or more) parasitic eggs are sometimes laid in the egg of the host. According to Marchal, such eggs develop independently, each producing a distinct polygerm and consequently a distinct brood. If the two eggs are of the same sex potentiality, the individuals developing from them will be either all females or all males, according to whether or not the eggs are fertilized or unfertilized. The dual origin of these double broods naturally elude detection in lots that have emerged. But if one of the two eggs is unfertilized and the other fertilized, the result will be a mixed brood, consisting of males and females. This conclusion of Marchal and Silvestri is strongly supported by the facts of polyembryonic development in the armadillos, in which it has been conclusively demonstrated (Fernandez, '09, Patterson, '13) that all of the embryos of a given pregnancy are the product of a single egg. As a result, mixed litters are never found in these mammals. That mixed broods may arise from two eggs in Copidosoma is supported by the fact that two polygerms are sometimes found in a single Gnorimoschema larva. However, certain facts concerning the condition of mixed broods in this species, make it doubtful whether the origin of all such broods can be explained in this obvious way. Careful dissections of something over a hundred parasitized Gnorimoschema larvae have revealed only two cases in which a single larva contained more than one polygerm. Since 6.17 per cent, of all broods are mixed, and since a similar number of unmixed broods would have a dual origin, we should expect to find over 12 per cent, of all parasitized larvae containing two polygerms, but instead, less than 2 per cent, are found. Another line of evidence which is not in harmony with the view that mixed broods are always the product of two or more eggs, is the great preponderance of females in certain lots. Of 356 J. T. PATTERSON. the nine complete lots (Broods 2 to 10) listed in Table V., the number of females in each case is greater than the number of males. In some cases (Broods 3, 4, 5, 7, 8), this difference is not so great but that the origin of each lot can be explained on the assumption that two eggs have been deposited in the egg of the host. But in Broods 2, 6, 9, and 10 the number of females in excess of males is indeed striking, making it difficult to explain the origin of such broods on the basis of two eggs. In view of these facts, the writer is convinced that some other explanation must be offered for the origin of certain mixed broods; in fact, one involving the idea that a single fertilized egg may give rise to a few males as well as a relatively large number of females. This would be possible on the basis of the following assumption. TABLE V. TABLE SHOWING THE NUMBER OF PARASITES IN MIXED BROODS. Brood. No. of Individuals. Females. Males. I* 89 20 69 2 162 153 9 3 172 92 80 4 207 126 81 5 216 176 40 6 235 223 12 7 241 161 80 8 300 235 65 9 304 292 12 10 337 3i6 21 Totals 2,26"? 1,704 460 Average 226.^5 I7O.4 46.9 * This brood is not complete, owing to the fact that some of the larvae and pupae had been destroyed by a large dipterous larva. If Copidosoma conforms to the general scheme for sex determin- ation in insects, the females must have the 2 X chromosomes, and males the single X chromosome. Ordinarily, during the process of cleavage, all of the chromosomes in the fertilized egg divide equally, so that all of the nuclei entering into the formation of the embryos will carry the XX chromosomes, thus producing a brood of females. But if during the early development of the egg it should happen that the two X chromosomes in one or more cleavages should not divide but separate, one going to each pole of the spindle, each daughter nucleus would then receive a single DEVELOPMENT OF COPIDOSOMA GELECHI^. 357 X chromosome. If such nuclei later divided in the typical manner and gave rise to embryos, such embryos would be males. One is encouraged to make this suggested explanation in the light of Bridges' ('13) discovery of the non-disjunction of the sex chromosomes in Drosophila. In Copidosoma the separation of the sex chromosomes during cleavage would be a case of "somatic" or "cleavage disjunction," while in Drosophila these chromosomes fail to separate or "disjoin" in the reduction division of the egg. In conclusion attention should be directed to the frequency of Copidosoma in nature. At Woods Hole about twenty per cent. of all Gnorimoschema larvae are infected with this parasite TABLE VI. TABLE SHOWING PERCENTAGE OF PARASITIZED CATERPILLARS IN THE GALLS OF SOLIDAGO SEMPERVIRENS. Number of Galls. Date. Parasitized by Copidosoma. Normal Galls Empty. Parasitized by Other Parasites. 9 7-29-12 7 2 0 O 33 8- S-I2 5 15 0 13 33 8-I7-I2 9 16 5 3 56 8- 8-12 7 26 10 13 29 8-1 2-1 2 8 16 o o 141 8-25-12 20 56 33 32 14 7- 7-13 i 13 o o 16 7-14-13 o 13 o o 39 7-15-13 8 31 0 0 38 7-19-13 4 * * * 23 7-23-13 2 20 o I 38 7-26-13 6 * * * 27 8- 5-13 4 * * * 24 8-25-13 4 17 3 0 18 6-15-14 2 14 2 o 19 6-18-14 3 16 O o 43 6-22-14 19 19 3 2 40 6-24-14 9 20 10 I 20 7-16-14 i 19 o 0 24 7-30-14 o 21 o 3 25 7-23-15 3 12 7 3 18 7-26-15 5 II o 2 66 7-30-15 25 37 2 2 35 8- 4-15 14 35 I 6 Totals. . 828 | 166 * Record incomplete. Copidosoma. About 20 per cent, of the caterpillars are parasitized by (Table VI.). As may be seen from the table, the extent of infection varies greatly in the lots of galls taken from different regions (those collected on a given date are all from a single locality). Plants which grow in exposed places, as along the 358 J. T. PATTERSON. roadside or barren spots, carry a higher percentage of galls than do those which are located in protected regions. Likewise, the moth larvae from the galls of the former are more highly parasitized. SUMMARY. 1. Copidosoma gelechiae, which is a parasite in the Solidago Gall Moth, Gnorimo schema salinaris, has but one generation a year. 2. The egg of this parasite is probably laid during the month of May. 3. The type of development in Copidosoma is polyembryonic. The number of individuals average about 191 per brood. 4. In the youngest stages secured the process of division of the egg into embryonic primordia is already in progress. The young polygerm consists of two distinct regions: (i) An outer zone, or nucleated membrane, containing the free polar . nuclei; (2) a central region, containing the true embryonic nuclei. 5. The embryonic nuclei segregate into groups, which become surrounded by a dense layer of granular protoplasm and form the primordia of the multiple embryos. 6. During early growth the polygerm elongates into a cylin- drical-shaped structure, which becomes broken up into several spherical, primary masses by the formation of constrictions in the nucleated membrane. Each primary mass receives several of the primitive embryos. 7. The primary masses become broken up into secondary masses by further constrictions of the nucleated membrane. At the end of these divisions, each embryo is separated from the others and is surrounded by an inner and an outer involucre — the former derived from the granular protoplasm and the latter from a portion of the nucleated membrane. 8. The interstices between these masses become filled with an inter-embryonal substance derived from at least three sources: elements from the nucleated membrane, leucocytes, and cells from the adipose tissue, which usually is laid down in the form of a thick layer on the outer surface of the polygerm. The entire structure thus becomes a complex, which may be called the polygermal mass. DEVELOPMENT OF COPIDOSOMA GELECHI^E. 359 9. The dissociation of the inter-embryonal substance sets the larvse free in the body cavity of the host. This occurs during the last week of July. 10. Abortive or degenerating embryos are found throughout the entire period covered by the polygerm and free larval stages. 1 1 . The free larvae destroy the entire contents of the caterpillar, except the chitinous parts of the trachae, and leave only the superficial layer of chitin of the body wall intact. The detritus left in the larval chitin hardens to form thin-walled, oval chambers in which the larvae lie and undergo pupation The superficial layer of chitin also hardens, and the larval skin thus becomes transformed into the typical mummified carcass, filled with the parasitic pupae. 13. Pupation takes place during the first ten days of August and lasts about a month. 14. The number of adult parasites emerging from the carcasses varies from 25 to 395. There is a preponderance of females, about 55 per cent, of all broods being females. Furthermore, the average number of females emerging from a single carcass is 198 as compared with 175 for the males. Ten mixed broods of males and females have been obtained. Some of these have doubtless arisen from two or more eggs; but it is suggested that such broods may also arise from a single fertilized egg, by a process of disjunction of the sex chromosomes during the early cleavage stages. WOODS HOLE, MASS., August 12, 1915. LITERATURE REFERENCES. Bridges, C. B. '13 Non-disjunction of the Sex Chromosomes of Drosophila. Jour, of Exp. Zool., Vol. XV., pp. 587-606. Fernandez, M. '09 Beitrage zur Embryologie der Gurteltiere. Morpholog. Jahrb., Bd. 39, PP- 302-333. Howard, L. O. '85 Description of North American Chalcididae. Bureau of Entomology Bulletin. Marchal, P. '98 Un exemple de dissociation de 1'oeuf. La cycle de 1'Encyrtus fuscicollis. C. R. Soc. Biol. Paris. T. 5. pp. 238-240. Marchal, P. '04 Recherches sur la Biologic et le Developpement des Hymenopteres Parasites. 360 J. T. PATTERSON. I. La Polyembryonie Specifique ou Germinogonie. Arch, de Zool. Exper. et Gen., Vol. II., pp. 257-335. Martin, F. '14 Zur Entvvicklungsgeschichte des polyembryonalen Chalcidiers Ageniaspis (Encyrtus) fuscicollis. Zeit. f. Wiss. Zool., Bd. no, pp. 419-479. Patterson, J. T. '13 Polyembryonie Development in Tatusia novemcincta. Jour. Morph., Vol. 24, pp. 559-684. Riley, C. V. '69 The Solidago Gall Moth. First Annual Report of the State Entomologist of Missouri, pp. 172-178. Sacken, Baron Osten. '63 Lasioptera Reared from a Gall on the Goldenrod. Proceed, of the Ent. Soc. of Phil. Vol. i, pp. 368-370. Silvestri, F. '06 Contribuzioni alia conoscenza biologica degli Immenotteri Parassiti. I. Biologia del Litomastix truncatellus Ann. d. Regia Scuola Superiore di Agricoltura di Portici, Vol. VI., pp. 1-51. Silvestri, F. '08 Contribuzioni alia conoscenza biologica degli Immenotteri Parassiti. II. Sviluppo dell'Ageniaspis fuscicollis. Ibid., Vol. VIII., pp. 1-27. Wheeler, W. M. '10 The Effects of Parasitic and other Kinds of Castration in Insects. Jour, of Exper. Zool., Vol. 8, pp. 377~438. 362 J. T. PATTERSON. DESCRIPTION OF PLATES. PLATE I. FIG. i. A typical gall of Gnorimoschema salinaris, Busck, situated at the base of the stalk of the swamp goldenrod, Solidago sempervirens. X /4- FIG. 2. Gall cut open to show the position of the mummified carcass of Gnori- moschema. Natural size. FIG. 3. Gall cut open and carcass removed to show the shape of cavity. Note that the walls of the cavity are smooth and that the excrement from the caterpillar is packed in the bottom of the cavity. Natural size. FIG. 4. Mummified carcass from gall shown in Fig. 3. Natural size. FIG. 5. Lepidopterous larva which is an inquiline in the gall of Gnorimoschema. Note the irregular shape of the cavity which contains scattered trash and excrement. Natural size. FIG. 6. This gall shows an incomplete passage-way, lying just above the head of the carcass. Normal size. FIG. 7. Side view of a gall showing the orifice of the passage-way, closed by silken plug. Natural size. FIG. 8. Stalk of swamp goldenrod containing two galls. X %• FIG. 9. Gall containing a non-parasitized caterpillar. Natural size. FIG. 10. Gall containing a parasitized caterpillar. Natural size. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL XXIX. PLATE I. J. T. PATTERSON. 364 J- T. PATTERSON. PLATE II. FIG. n. Photomicrograph of a section of an irregular polygermal mass. X40. FIG. 12. Photomicrograph of a single embryo from mass shown in next figure. Xi8o. FIG. 13. Photomicrograph of a longitudinal section of a flat, plate-like poly- germal mass. X 40. FIG. 14. Photomicrograph of a spherical polygermal mass which is barren of adipose tissue. X 40. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXIX. PLATE II. J. T. PATTERSON. 366 J. T. PATTERSON. PLATE III. FIG. 15. Photomicrograph of the middle portion of longitudinal section of a small caterpillar. A fat body containing a polygerm lies just below the intestine. X44- FIG. 1 6. Photomicrograph of a portion of a section of a polygermal mass which was about to begin disintegration. X 44. FIG. 17. Photomicrograph of a section of a polygermal mass undergoing dis- sociation. X 44. FIG. 1 8. Photomicrograph of a mass of free larvae from the body cavity of the caterpillar. Note that each embryo is still surrounded by the involucres. X 44. Reference Letters Used in Plates IV. -VI. A.E., Abortive Embryo. I.S., Inter-embryonal Substance. A.T., Adipose Tissue. N.M., Nucleated Membrane. C., Clear space left by contraction of O.I., Outer Involucre. Precipitated Material. P.D., Primary Division of polygerm. E.N., Embryonic Nuclei. P.E., Primitive Embryo. G.L., Granular Layer. P.M., Precipitated Material. /./., Inner Involucre. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXIX. PLATE III. . •_••.•»«.. „" .-•--.-._ . ' • '• • '• 16 15 I 17 18 J T. PATTERSON. 368 J. T. PATTERSON. PLATE IV. FIG. 19. A and B longitudinal sections of two polygerms lying close together in the same caterpillar. These polygerms show an early phase of the segregation of the embryonic nuclei to form the separate embryos. X 489. FIG. 20. Section of an egg of Gnorimoschema which has started to develop parthenogenetically. X 173. FIG. 21. Longitudinal section of a polygerm showing the end phase of embryo formation. X 480. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXIX. PLATE IV. J. T. PATTERSON. 37° J- T. PATTERSON. PLATE V. FIG. 22. One end of a longitudinal section of a polygerm showing three of the twelve primary divisions into which it has been divided by constrictions of the nucleated membrane. X 373. FIG. 23. Section of a primary mass showing the process by which it is further divided up into secondary masses by constrictions of the nucleated membrane. X 508. FIG. 24. Section of a single isolated, primary mass about at the close of its division into single embryonic masses. X 257. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXIX. PLATE V. \» ^ • ,jj »!»*• - -«*«j«" - r~, — k *. j-» , . • J; i. .1'. ' J*. '..L^*T\ ". -.-.\ ^v«_ KS^a^S^^B &&. x- ,^-r ---'?; rL^vr*:^ .. WM^ jpg;^ ,^-vJ^ ^(:^:^|^^^. ft-a^^l*, f,:^^"1 , I ^*^1 *'- ^sS:^ A{ J. T. PATTERSON. 23 G. Z. ^ •.r--. -• ,i.{ tr2^© 24 • - o 372 J. T. PATTERSON. PLATE VI. FIG. 25. A to H, Series of sketches from Lot III of the free larvae listed in Table III. This figure shows the great variation in size of the larvae from a single caterpillar. They are all drawn to the same scale. FIG. 26. Detailed drawing of a section of one of the embryos seen in Fig. 13. It shows the relation of the inter-embryonal substance and involucres to the embryo. X 187. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXIX. PLATE VI. J. T. PATTERSON. DISTRIBUTION OF FOLLICULINA IN 1914. E. A. ANDREWS. The finding of vast hordes of the Stentor-\ike infusorian Follicu- lina both in 1912 and 1913 throughout the whole extent of the Severn River which is a brackish side branch of the Chesapeake Bay, led to further examination in 1914 to see if this were a phenomenon to be repeated annually or only a rare inroad of an outside fauna into new territory. In I9I31 FolHculina'was found in inconceivable numbers living upon the leaves of the fresh water plants Elodea and Potamogeton, which have taken possession of definite zones of shallow brackish water along some fifty and more miles of extent of the river and its side creeks. It was also found on Elodea in Whitehall River, just to the north of the Severn. In 1914 it was taken on Elodea from the head of the Magothy River, August 13, and on floating Elodea in the mouth of the Magothy, August 23, when it was also found living upon stunted Elodea growing in the narrow inlet canal to the nearly shut off side branch known as the Little Magothy. It was taken also at Deep Creek, a side branch of the Magothy. As the Magothy opens into the Chesapeake some seven miles from the Severn, the distribution of Folliculina is quite extensive. Moreover, in 1880 Ryder2 found Folliculina in great numbers upon oyster shells in shallow water on the west coast of the Chesapeake, and as he seems to have then been at St. Jerome, St. Mary's County, which is sixty miles down the Bay from the Severn, the distribution of Folliculina is known for side branches of the Bay opening into it seventy miles apart, approximately. It is to be expected then that exceedingly large areas of the side waters of the Chesapeake may be inhabited by this little- known protozoan, which in the mid-summer season adds greatly 1 See BIOL. BUI.L., XXVI., No. 4, April, 1914. 2 Am. Nat., 14, 1880. 373 374 E- A- ANDREWS. to the plankton, or swimming fauna, as well as to the microscopic life attached to the summer vegetation of these waters. Its advent and departure in Chase's Creek, a branch of the Severn, showed in 1914 even more suddenness than in 1913, while its time of abundance was noticeably less though actual numbers present were even more vast. Though searched for from the middle of June, every few days, Folliculina was found first on July 19, 1914. It then appeared only here and there, not on every plant of Elodea and on very few plants of Potamogeton. On the sprays of Elodea the Folliculina showed on comparatively few leaves, like black soot stuck on the leaves; both isolated individuals and aggregates occurred i It *# FIG. i. Leaf of Potamogeton showing scattered colonies of Folliculina. X3 diam. Photograph of preserved specimen. but there were very few large aggregates covering half the surface of a single leaf. Most leaves had none, some leaves had many scattered individuals. On the stems there were noticeable numbers of the small form of sac. The occurrence on leaves seemed entirely arbitrary as if from settlements of swimmers: the Folliculina was not now crowded toward the tips of the sprays but scattered along many inches of the spray. At the date of this first appearance, jellyfish had been common for two weeks but the other conspicuous summer visitor to these waters, the young menhaden now for the first time came along the shores over the Elodea, which may be correlated with the DISTRIBUTION OF FOLLICULINA IN 1914. 375 feeding of the menhaden upon plankton in which the free swim- ming Folliculina may be included as possible food for the men- haden. At this date the Elodea had grown up to a height of twenty inches and formed some flower stalks and buds at the surface, so that there had been a long period in which suitable attachment base for Folliculina was present but the Folliculina had been absent. July 21 the water after long drought was turbid from the presence of plankton and the Folliculina had increased but little, appearing as black spots on one out of several hundred sprays of Elodea and one out of many thousands of Potamogeton sprays. Only a few of the leaves on each inhabited spray had dense aggregates, so that the question arises: why do the Folliculina FIG. 2. Tip of leaf of Elodea covered with a colony of Folliculina. X IS diam. Photograph of preserved specimen. crowd together in these rare, isolated aggregates? When sprays L of these dates were put into aquaria they gave rise to free swim- ming forms, thus showing that these early settlers need not remain fixed but might contribute to additional distributions. On July 27 Folliculina had become much more abundant upon sprays of Elodea and Potamogeton; some of the free-floating frag- ments on the surface appeared black with the accumulated 376 E. A. ANDREWS. Folliculina. In the water also some free-swimming Folliculina could be seen near the surface swimming all through the water as well as close to floating plants. Out in the Severn River a two-quart jar of water taken up at random at the surface showed several free-swimming Folliculina; FIG. 3. Photograph of a preserved colony that had been formed on surface of the water in aquarium ; showing form of case and tube spirals as well as animal rectracted within case. Enlarged 30 diameters. three days later these had settled down on the side of the jar and were in two groups, two individuals in one and five in the other, so that at least seven were in the two quarts of surface water, which would make an immense number for the entire river. DISTRIBUTION OF FOLLICULINA IN 377 By August i much of the Elodea growing in the Elodea zone along shore was black with aggregates of Folliculina. Free swimmers were in the water of the creek in vast numbers : a quart dipped from the surface at random showed in a white bowl from fourteen to one hundred, by actual count, for each quart of water from the surface. By drawing the bowl along the surface, the Folliculina swimming free were concentrated till thousands in a quart made it dark as if sprinkled with black pepper. Though these free-swimming Folliculinas easily escape notice in the greenish water turbid with plankton and sediment, they are readily observed in calm water by an eye near the surface; and standing in water five feet deep one may see them swimming FIG. 4. Photograph of two young colonies of free swimmers that have just settled on surface of water in aquarium and formed sacs but no tubes: one individual on extreme left is still in motile form. Preserved specimen, X 20 diam. rapidly in all directions, individually in straight and in curved paths. Many deep down in the water were seen best by holding a white object below them, but most of them were near the surface where they congregated especially about any floating object as fallen leaf or floating chip, seemingly influenced by its presence so that they swam toward it. While at this time the Folliculina continued to colonize the new growths at tip of the Elodea as fast as it grew so that the 378 E. A. ANDREWS. black aggregates crowded on the young leaves nearly to the tip where only the newest leaves were as yet unoccupied ; by August 1 8 the extension of the Folliculina hosts had ceased. The tips of the growing Elodea were now bare or free from Folliculina back some twenty leaves from the tip and many of the old dwel- lings on the lower leaves were deserted. These dense black colonies on old leaves contained in fact but few living Folliculinas. FIG. 5. Photograph of natural size sprays of Elodea preserved to show successive phases of colonization in 1914. Spray on left has grown enough to form flower but as yet but a very few .isolated individual Folliculina have settled upon it. The next spray shows scattered tubes all along its length. The third spray shows dense aggregations of colonies even up to the tips of the rapidly unfolding new leaves. The fourth spray illustrates the subsidence in colonization: the new colonies no longer cover the leaves at the tip of the spray but these grow more rapidly than the new colonists occupy them and are left more nearly free from any Folliculinas. By August 26 this falling off in the colonization and rapid dis- appearance of Folliculina was most pronounced : the Elodea sprays showed an abrupt transition from the lower leaves black from dense population of tubes, for the most part empty, to the upper leaves only sparsely inhabited with scattered individuals. Evi- dently some sudden change had operated not only to check the previously rapid spread of the Folliculinas onto new leaves but to DISTRIBUTION OF FOLLICULINA IN 1914. 379 almost exterminate them. Yet many remained alive here and there so that when large quantities of the Elodea were put into aquaria many free swimmers escaped. Yet these after forming new tubes on the surface of the water did not remain alive but had all vanished September 5, though in such apparently normal environment others had been kept two weeks in captivity earlier in the season. Thus while appearing after the middle of July and being extra- ordinarily abundant in August, the Folliculina were all gone about the end of August and no way was found of keeping them longer. Their period of existence in accessible regions of the river was scarcely six weeks. In 1913 they appeared before the end of June and a few lingered on to the first of September in nature and were kept in aquaria in a warm room till the 2/th and a few till November 11. In 1912 no live ones were found after September 8. This enormous crowding of the waters with free-swimming Folliculina and dense settlements of the case-making Folliculinas during about a month, the last weeks of July and the first of August, coincides with very high temperatures and abundance of micro- scopic plankton in these waters but it is not at all evident either why the Folliculinas should not come earlier, as they did in 1913, or remain later as they did in 1913 and 1912. The great rapidity of their colonization of large areas suggests either very great immigration or else very rapid multiplication, or combination of both. As all material searched in the daytime in 1913 failed to show more than a few cases of multiplication, most all the free-swimming forms being merely the case-making forms again freed, material was collected at all times of the night in 1914, but here again but few cases of division were observed. Hence it seems unlikely that fission of a few immigrants actually produced the vast numbers found on the leaves of plants, and it is probable that very large numbers came into the river suddenly from some outside source and these settling down, migrating out again, and in some cases increasing by fission, gave rise to the succession of dwellings covering the leaves for some two months. The causes leading to the immigration as well as the causes of rather sudden diminution of numbers and utter disappearance remain entirely unknown. 380 E. A. ANDREWS. The food of the case-inhabiting Folliculina being bacteria and some larger forms of plankton, the disappearance of Folliculina may well be associated with changes in food supply, in turn brought about in connection with such changes as those of tem- perature and salinity. The motile forms take no food and may be enabled to settle and to continue migration and multiplication only when feeding conditions allow the sessile form to accumulate enough energy. SUMMARY. 1. The vast swarms of swimming protozoans of the genus Folliculina that were found to settle down over the aquatic plants along the shores of side branches of the Chesapeake Bay in 1912 and 1913, came in even greater numbers in 1914, and it is there- fore probable that this immigration and colonization is a regular annual phenomenon. 2. The incursions of swimming Folliculina do not take place as soon as the plants have grown enough to supply places for attachment, and the departure or disappearance of the living Folliculinas antedates the cessation of growth and final dying down of the plants upon which they settle. 3. As far as evidence is available the numbers that crowd the leaves arise more from immigration from without the area than from division of animals that have already settled in the area. 4. The times of appearance and disappearance differ in suc- cessive years. 5. It is suggested that conditions of food possibilities are determining factors in these inroads into the brackish fauna. 6. The great number of free swimming forms makes them, for the time being, an important factor in the plankton. 7. The crowding of the dwellings or cases on the leaves all along the shores is a considerable element in the transformation of matter which, arising from decay of organic materials, is trans- formed into bacteria and other plankton organisms, which in turn are eaten by Folliculina and enable them to secrete resisting tubes and sacs which finally settle to the bottom of the river. PHENOMENA OF ORIENTATION EXHIBITED BY EPHEMERID/E.1 F. H. KRECKER. It is a well-known fact that in alighting Ephemeridsc orient positively to a breeze. I became interested in this reaction and the observations made naturally lead to others on reactions to gravity and to light, and to the results of a conflict between any of these three stimuli. The observations were made during the summer of 1915 at the Lake Laboratory of Ohio State University at Cedar Point on Lake Erie. Ephemeridse appear here in almost incredible numbers. When a brood is at its height it is a very common occurrence to find piles of the insects three or four feet square and six to eight inches deep undenelectric lights. At a neighboring amusement resort several carts were required each morning to haul away the dead insects. The species with which the following observations are especially concerned is Hexagenia variabilis. The number, variety and arrangement of lights at the resort presented favorably conditions for observing the reactions to light of great numbers of individuals in what may be termed natural surroundings. The equipment used for experiments with air currents and gravity was simple and largely improvised. Never- theless, since it is not primarily my purpose to measure intensity of stimuli or rapidity of reaction, I believe the results obtained have some interest and value. REACTIONS TO A CURRENT OF AIR. There was a question in my mind as to whether the positive orientation of the Ephemeridee to a breeze is a response to the breeze per se or whether other factors are concerned. In order to test this I took a piece of glass tubing several inches long and sent through it a weak but steady current of air so directed 1 Contribution from the Department of Zoology and Entomology, Ohio State University, No. 43. 381 382 F. H. KRECKER. as to strike the insects on the side of the body. They were resting on boards placed horizontally. A few of them flew away but most of them eventually faced the current. Individuals placed on a rough surface, such as a wire screen, which afforded a better foothold frequently tried to walk away. When facing the current of air an individual would raise its long, slender front pair of legs and extend them forward and upward at an angle of about 40 degrees. When held in this way the legs re- semble antennae and it is possible they have a sensory function. However, cutting them off had no apparent effect on the reactions here in question. The time required for the turning reaction varied from an almost instantaneous response to two minutes. In the majority of cases the response was gradual and occupied from 30 seconds to one minute. The rapidity of reaction de- pended upon a correlation between the strength of the breeze and the part of the body it struck. The influence of the area stimulated is shown in experiments with the wings. The latter are large in proportion to the body and meet over the back in a perpendicular position. They, therefore, present quite a broad surface. When a current of air of an intensity sufficient to blow the wings slightly to one side was directed against them individuals would react in fifteen to thirty seconds, whereas when this current was directed against the thorax or the abdomen the response was slower, if indeed any occurred. A stronger current directed against any of these parts brought about a correspondingly more rapid reaction. In another series of experiments a current of air was directed from the posterior lengthwise of the body along the dorsal surface of a number of individuals. The response in these cir- cumstances was also an eventual facing about to the current. A current of air striking an individual longitudinally along the mid-dorsal surface is neutral so far as lateral directions are con- cerned. In the cases here in question the current blew the wings to one side or the other and then as before the insects turned around toward the side on which the strain was exerted. The experiments were repeated on a group of individuals from which the wings had been removed. The results from a current of air striking the insects on the side of the body were the same PHENOMENA OF ORIENTATION EXHIBITED BY EPHEMERIDyE. 383 as before; the insects faced the current. However, when a current was directed from the rear longitudinally along the dorsal surface of the body the previous results were not repeated. In some cases the insects crawled with the current and away from the point of origin. In other cases they remained stationary and took an attitude similar to that assumed when facing the current. If the current became very strong they either attempted to crawl away or they retained the attitude until blown off their feet. When the current veered sufficiently to strike them on the side they began to turn toward it. In these experiments with air currents the first noticeable response from the insects was an attempt to hold on to the surface upon which they were resting. This they did by fastening their claws firmly and even changing the position of the legs. When the current became so strong as to make it difficult to remain attached and especially when the body was blown over to one side the insects began to change position, rather hesitatingly it appeared, and to face about toward the direction from which the current came. When an insect reached a position where it did not seem to have difficulty in maintaining its hold it came to rest. This usually meant that it was directly facing the current, al- though sometimes it stopped at a point between a half and a com- plete about face. A half about face could generally be made complete by increasing the strength of the current. When directly facing a current of air an individual is in the optimum position for resistance; it presents the least surface and the claws because of their backward curve have the maximum effect in holding the body. On the other hand when an individual stands sidewise to the current a greater surface is presented, the claws are not in a relatively favorable position and attach- ment is clearly more difficult. WTith regard to the more rapid reactions which result when the current strikes the wings it may be said that the proportionately great expanse of the wings above the body's center of gravity gives them such a leverage that the body is more easily tipped over, a strain is more quickly felt and attachment more quickly made difficult. In those cases in which a current struck wingless individuals from the posterior there was practically no obstruction to the current 384 F. H. KRECKER. and it consequently did not so easily cause strain or seriously disturb the attachment and there was therefore no turning reaction. It would appear from the foregoing experiments that the Ephemeridae do not change position under the stimulus of a breeze until a strain is exerted on the organs of attachment. That this does not merely mean that the response was delayed, until a breeze of a given intensity developed is shown by the fact that a comparatively weak breeze directed against the wings alone had the same effect as was caused by a somewhat stronger breeze against the thorax. There is, therefore, evidence, I believe, for concluding that Ephemeridae do not orient positively to a breeze because of sensations derived from the breeze per se but that they react positively to tension exerted on the muscles of attachment. REACTIONS TO GRAVITY. The position of Ephemeridae when resting upon a perpendicular surface is negative with regard to the earth's surface and usually approximately vertical to it, although variations as great as 45 degrees occur. On comparatively smooth surfaces the orien- tation is more generally an approximation to the vertical, whereas on surfaces such as a wire screen, which affords a good foothold at any angle, variations from the vertical may occur in 50 per cent, of the individuals concerned. Individuals picked up by the wings and replaced head downward, if they are not so disturbed as to fly away, will struggle to gain a foothold. The position of the claws, which are adapted to a vertical position, make attachment rather difficult. This difficulty is increased by the fact that the long abdomen is thrown forward and down- ward and thus tends to destroy equilibrium. On comparatively smooth surfaces such as a planed board the insects rarely suc- ceeded in maintaining their equilibrium long enough to gain a footing. On a wire screen they were more often successful and once they gained a footing and their equilibrium they retained the new position. The picking up process caused so many of the insects to fly away that other methods were tried. Several in- dividuals were placed in a vertical position on a straw hat held perpendicularly and then the hat was slowly revolved until the PHENOMENA OF ORIENTATION EXHIBITED BY EPHEMERID/E. 385 insects were upside down. The overhanging abdomen disturbed the equilibrium of some of them sufficiently to cause them to lose their hold and fly off. The others retained their footing, in some cases by changing the position of the legs, and remained in the inverted position for ten to fifteen minutes which was as long as they were watched. In explanation of the position normally assumed on an upright surface the evidence derived from the experiments seems to indicate that the position taken is not a negative reaction to gravity per se but that it is largely, if not entirely, due to the character of the insect's means of attachment. Results obtained from experiments performed to test the in- fluence of a breeze upon the position of the insects on a perpen- dicular surface support this view. A current of air was directed against the side of individuals resting in the normal upright position on a perpendicular surface. As they turned the current was so directed as to bring them still further around. During the process some of them could not retain their foothold and flew off. The others turned completely around and faced directly downward. They maintained the inverted position at least as long as they were under observation, ten to fifteen minutes, which length of time, in view of a constant coming and going among those normally situated, seemed sufficient. REACTION TO LIGHT. The conclusions with regard to reactions of the Ephemeridse to light are largely the result of observations made in the amuse- ment resort already mentioned. The observations have to do mostly with artificial light. The insects react negatively to bright sunlight and seek the shade. They are strongly attracted to the lighter colors of artificial light. In the resort there are a great many electric lights of sixteen candle power intensity with colorless glass bulbs. Many of them are attached in a horizontal position to the sides of buildings in such a way that there is a perpendicular surface either above or below them and frequently on all sides. The reaction to these lights seems to be satisfied if the insects can come to rest within a zone which begins approximately six inches from the light and covers a radius extending outward for F. H. KRECKER. about twenty-four to thirty inches. When individuals enter this optimum zone they alight, if a surface is available, and orient themselves in such a way that the body is parallel with a radius projecting from the light. After alighting the insects usually remain at rest, although there may be a certain amount of crawling toward a position nearer the center. This is more often done by those nearer the outer limits of the zone. When the insects are numerous they become arranged in rows consisting of individuals either directly behind one another or slightly to one side and they thus form a striking pattern of radiating lines. -;-vV;-', ^ s \ i ^ \ ^ i ' , / • / / ; FIG. i. The accompanying figures illustrate the positions assumed with regard to lights in different positions and combinations. The first figure illustrates the position assumed when the surface extends about a light in all directions whether the plane be horizontal or vertical. When any portion of the surface is absent the pattern is of course interrupted to a corresponding extent. The clear zone immediately surrounding the light was approximately six inches wide. I shall call it the excitement zone. Individuals that entered this zone became greatly excited and fluttered about the light in a confused state. There was no evidence to show that individuals at rest deliberately entered the excitement- zone. Those immediately bordering on it were rather restless and occasionally in crawling about some were pushed into it and others on taking wing came within the influence of the light. PHENOMENA OF ORIENTATION EXHIBITED BY EPHEMERID/E. 387 The second figure shows lights arranged along the lower edge of a perpendicular surface at intervals of twelve to fifteen inches. About each light was the usual excitement zone and upward from this extended the radiating lines of insects in the optimum zone. As shown in the diagram these lines were rarely at an angle of less than 35 degrees. This was due to the fact that below this point the lines from neighboring lights conflicted and caused such confusion among the insects as to obliterate regular alignment. The greatest confusion occurred in the comparatively short space \ \ \ \ / / ' / / / ' ' < ',', '/ ^ NO ''/ / '.' / ,V I V N^ XX '// FIG. 2. between the lights where insects attempting to arrange them- selves about one light constantly came into conflict with others attracted to the neighboring light. When the insects rested on a horizontal plane about a light they faced it. The most striking feature connected with the arrangement of the insects on a perpendicular surface was that the individuals on opposite sides of a horizontal plane passing through the center of a light had opposite ends of the body directed toward the light. The insects below the plane or parallel with it faced the light, whereas those that were above the plane were turned away from the light. In other words all the insects? except those parallel with the horizontal plane, approximated a vertical position with the anterior end uppermost. Those above the plane and with the posterior end directed toward the light were apparently as well content as those below the plane and facing the light. The position of the insects on a horizontal surface shows that other things being equal they face the light. It is reasonable to conclude that their normal reaction to light is positive. The negative position assumed on a perpendicular surface above a light can be explained, in view of the air current and the inversion experiments, as being due to the difficulty experienced in main- taining a foothold in the inverted position. 388 F. H. KRECKER. Some observations were also made on the relative influence of white and colored lights. On the sides of one of the buildings in the resort there was a succession of alternating white, red and blue lights. The slightly yellowish white bulb attracted the insects in greatest numbers. There was the usual excitement zone and the regular alignment of those at rest. The number of insects about the red and the blue bulbs was decidedly small and as between the two lights about the same. These lights appeared to have a quieting effect on the insects. The alignment was similar to that described for white lights but there was no well- defined excitement zone, in fact the insects crawled about the bulbs without exhibiting markedly abnormal reactions. OHIO STATE UNIVERSITY, COLUMBUS, OHIO. CELL MULTIPLICATION IN THE SUB-CUTICULA OF DILEPIS SCOLECINA.1 DALTON G. PAXMAN. INTRODUCTION. The process of cell division in cestodes as compared with that in other Metazoa is apparently quite abnormal. An examination of cestode material at once reveals the fact that mitotic figures are very rare, and that an explanation of the process of cell division analogous to any of the common types is apparently impossible. The opinion of the various workers in cestode cy- tology, as to how cell division is taking place, varies greatly. Some state that it occurs by mitosis, others by amitosis, while it has been asserted that nuclei arise ' de novo ' from the cytoplasm. Child ('07) noted the apparent infrequency or total absence of any evidence of mitosis in Moniezia, even in regions where rapid growth was taking place. He says, "If my observations are correct, amitosis is the more common method of division in the generative cycle, except during the period of maturation and early cleavage. And in the somatic cells of the adult body it appears to be the usual method at all times." Young ('08), working with Cysticercus pisiformis describes what he calls the "de novo" formation of cells. He observed irregular masses of coarsely granular cytoplasm lying in the meshes of the parenchyma network. These masses contain numerous small deep staining granules scattered haphazard through the mass. Shortly succeeding the formation of these granules, a nuclear membrane is formed around them; the newly formed nucleus, together with a small mass of cytoplasm, becomes partly constricted from the parent mass; and the daughter cell has been formed." Further, he says: "I believe that the nucleus in these forms is not a morphological, but a physiological entity; that the 1 A thesis presented to the graduate faculty of the University of North Dakota in partial fulfilment of the requirements for a master's degree. 389 39O DALTON G. PAXMAN. nuclear granules are fundamentally the same as the remaining protoplasm of the cell, but are differentiated therefrom under physiological conditions which we do not at present understand; that the granules are perhaps reserve material stored up in the nucleus for future use, the entire cell body being thus occasionally converted into a nucleus; and the nucleus varies in structure from time to time in response to the varing physiological demands made upon it. ... Further if my interpretation of my ob- servations be correct, then distinction between germ and somatic plasm is obviously impossible, a special vehicle for the trans- ference of hereditary qualities is entirely wanting; such qualities must be transmitted by the undifferentiated protoplasm; cell lineage is manifestly lacking; a mosaic theory is plainly untenable; and the fate of any given embryonic element — whether it shall form parenchyma, muscle, nerve, etc. — must be determined by physiological causes alone." Richards (1911), working with Moniezia, does not agree with Child. He says (p. 158): "I have after diligent search upon carefully prepared material been unable to establish a series of stages in the autoconstriction and subsequent division of the nucleus and cell body by amitosis. Considering the evidence as set forth, it seems to the writer that one is forced to the con- clusion that mitosis is the method by which pre-oogonia and cleavage divisions are accomplished." Mary T. Harman ('13, p. 223) states: "My observations have not shown that amitosis does not take place in Taenia or Moniezia, but they have showrn no condition which cannot be as readily explained as the result of mitotic as of amitotic division." MATERIALS AND PROCEDURE. The form I worked with was Dilepis scolecina parasitic in the small intestine of the double-crested cormorant (Phalocrocorax dilophus). These birds are found abundantly near the shores and on the islands of Devils Lake, North Dakota. Immediately after the bird was killed, the cestodes were removed from the intestine and placed in fixing solution. Flem- ming's solution and cestode mixture were the fixatives used. Flemming's solution blackened the tissue so that the results CELL MULTIPLICATION IN SUB-CUTICULA. 39! from it were not satisfactory. The cestode mixture, however, gave excellent results. The stains used were the following: Heidenhain's iron-alum- haematoxylin without counterstain ; safranin counterstained with light green; thionin counterstained with acid fuchsin; methyl green counterstained with acid fuchsin; and safranin counter- stained with water blue. OBSERVATIONS. I began my study of cell multiplication in cestodes without any previous knowledge of what had been done in the field of cestode cytology. Moreover, I completed the study of my material and drew my conclusions before I read any of the literature on the subject. I have confined my study of cell multiplication in Dilepis to the sub-cuticula. In this tissue I have searched in vain for a single clear case of mitosis or amitosis. Moreover, in order to be certain I had not overlooked any, I counted 10,000 resting nuclei in the sub-cuticula of the neck regions of ten worms with the same result. Certainly active growth must have been taking place in this region, but it could not be accounted for by mitotic or amitotic division. I have, however, observed numerous places in this region in which active cell multiplication was apparently taking place. Here multinucleate cells, such as shown in Fig. i, have been observed. In addition to these, large protoplasmic masses were present, which varied in size from that of a single cell to that of perhaps fifty cells massed together. Fig. 2 shows a typical mass. These masses stain rather deeply with nuclear stains, and contain from one to five nuclei. These masses are found abundantly in the neck region of every worm I examined, and occur, although less frequently, in the body region. By reference to any of these figures it is seen at once that the mass of cytoplasm is out of proportion to the mass of the nuclei. Moreover, I have observed numerous lobes and occasionally even entire masses in which I was unable to find any trace of a distinct nucleus. Fig. 7 shows a lobe,1 i, and Fig. 6 a mass of 1 At focal levels other than that shown in the figure the lobe was seen to be continuous with nucleate masses. DALTON G. PAXMAN. protoplasm, h, in which no well-defined nucleus is present. How- ever, in this latter case the mass is so close to a nucleate mass that I cannot say positively that it is not continuous with it. By closely examining the nuclei present in these masses, I find that the nuclear membranes are very indistinct in many cases. Fig. 2 shows a mass in which the nuclei have indistinct membranes. Also one of the nuclei, c, has a somewhat less distinct membrane than the other, b. And this latter membrane is in turn less distinct than the membranes of the nuclei in the cell syncytium above it. Moreover, a large number of nuclei have been seen which lack membranes completely. The nucleus consisted of a "nucleolus" or "karyosome" surrounded by a clear zone. Figs. 3, 4, and 5 show "karyosomes" which lack membranes. As Child and Young have already suggested, I believe this " nu- cleolus" represents the chromatin material of the nucleus. By observing the protoplasm under high magnification (2,000 diameters) it is seen that the protoplasmic strands contain many dark staining granules of various sizes and shapes. Some of these granules were as large as the "nucleoli " of the complete nuclei ; others, however, were so small as to be scarcely discernible. Fig. 4 shows a mass which contains a number of varying-sized granules. Fig. 5 shows a mass which contains a number of varying-sized granules one of which, g, is becoming surrounded by a clear zone. The protoplasmic masses apparently arise by the outgrowth of protoplasm from certain cells of the syncytium. Figs. 2, 3, 4, and 6, show masses of protoplasm continuous with the syncytial cells around them. In Fig. 6, the developing mass is very small and contains no definite nucleus. In Figs. 2, 3, and 4, the masses are very large and contain from one to five complete nuclei. A large number of masses have been observed varying in size between these extremes. The nuclear membranes of the nuclei in the cells from which these masses are developing, contain very small, irregular granules which stain darkly like the granules in the cytoplasm. I have insufficient evidence for or against Young's view of the "de novo" origin of these granules. The chromatin granules may arise "de novo" in the cytoplasm and CELL MULTIPLICATION IN SUB-CUTICULA. 393 develop to complete nuclei in situ. Young bases his theory of the independent origin of granules from a cytogenic protoplasmic mass upon the following facts: 1. The occurrence of masses of granular protoplasm lacking any evident nuclei. 2. The occurrence of isolated " nucleoli" of varying size from 34 to i micron in diameter, which are usually found in the above mentioned masses of protoplasm but occasionally lie free in the parenchyma strands. I believe, however, that these facts may be equally well ac- counted for by assuming the extrusion of chromidia from a mother nucleus. Masses of granular protoplasm without any evident nuclei, which occur but rarely may be explained as having been severed from parent masses after impregnation \vith chromidia. The occurrence of isolated "nucleoli" can be ac- counted for just as well by assuming the migration of chromidia from the nuclei along the strands of the cytoplasmic network, as by the assumption of their development from the protoplasm in situ. Young, in a later paper ('13) dealing with gametogenesis, in Tcenia pisiformis says (p. 375): "I believe that new nuclei arise either from chromidial extrusions from old nuclei, or 'de novo' in the cytoplasm. . . . The structure of the nucleus — a loose collection of chromatin bodies without a membrane — renders the extrusion of chromidia an easy matter. After their extrusion new chromatin is added and that part of the cell containing them is constricted off, to give rise in its turn to other cells. ... It is obviously impossible to say, however, whether any chromatin granule in the cytoplasm is a chromidial extrusion or a 'de novo' formation." Since I have seen these very small granules, all of about the same size, present in the nuclear membrane as though impeded by it in their exit, along the strands of the protoplasmic network, from the nucleus to the cytoplasm, I believe that these granules are extruded from the mother nucleus. Moreover, since I have observed granules of various shapes and sizes, many of the larger ones appearing to be composed of three or four smaller ones partly united, and since I have often seen a number of 394 DALTON G. PAXMAN. granules clustered together, I believe that the larger granules are the result of the union of many smaller ones. Thus, I believe that the small particles of chromatin or "chromidia" are ex- truded from the mother nucleus. Then these "chromidia" unite here and there throughout the protoplasm to form larger granules or "karyosomes" which become surrounded by a clear zone. Finally the nuclear membrane is formed, producing a daughter nucleus. When a number of nuclei have been formed multinucleate cells are the result. Since the tissue is always a cell syncytium, constrictions of the cytoplasm around a nucleus finish the production of a daughter cell. Thus one mother cell may produce a large number of daughter cells. COMPARISON WITH T^ENIA PISIFORMIS. In order to compare the process of cell multiplication in Dilepis with that in other cestodes, Dr. Young has permitted me to examine his slides of Tcenia pisiformis, and Cysticercus pisiformis. Here I have identified the protoplasmic masses in both the adult and the larva. These also contain nuclei in the various stages of formation from chromidia to complete nuclei. The young larvae show large numbers of protoplasmic masses developing in the cell syncytium. In the older larvae the masses often show four or five nuclei developing membranes at the same time. DISCUSSION. Cell multiplication by means of protoplasmic masses and the development of nuclei from chromidia, has, so far as I am aware, never been observed heretofore in Metazoa by anyone except Young. He has described the process as it occurs in Cysticercus pisiformis (Young, '08) and has noted it in some other cestodes (Young, '10) although his interpretation varies slightly from my own. I have, in the present paper given an account of it as it occurs in the sub-cuticula of Dilepis scolecina. It is true that chromidia have been observed in certain Metazoa, but no account of their functioning in the reproduction of the cell has ever been given previous to Young's paper on the " Histogenesis of Cy- sticercus pisiformis." If cells are actually developing from protoplasmic masses in CELL MULTIPLICATION IN SUB-CUTICULA. 395 the manner described, we have here an exceptional method of cell multiplication, unlike anything previously described in Metazoa.1 Moreover, if future research supports this view, the present theories of the role of the nucleus in heredity will have to be greatly modified at least with respect to cestodes. As Young has previously suggested, the explanation of such a method of cell multiplication as this may rest on the fact that the cestode is highly degenerate in most characteristics due to its long period of parasitism. In the development of cells from protoplasmic masses the nucleus passes through a cycle in which occur stages resembling nuclei of lower forms. The protoplasmic mass with its diffused nuclei in the form of chromidia is com- parable to a cell of the Bacteria or of the Myxophycese. In certain Protozoa also, as noted by many observers, the nuclear material at certain periods diffuses throughout the cytoplasm in the form of chromidia which may give origin to secondary nuclei, and these in turn to gametes. It is possible that the cestode nucleus has lost the power of mitotic division, accom- panying the somatic degeneration of the worm due to parasitism. Richards, Harman, and others have shown, however, that we still find cell division taking place by mitosis in the sex cells and developing embryos. CONCLUSIONS. I have made the following conclusions in regard to cell multi- • plication in the sub-cuticula of Dilepis scolecina. 1. After a careful examination, and after counting 10,000 of the nuclei in this region, I conclude that the growth of the sub- cuticula cannot be accounted for by mitotic or amitotic division. 2. Tissue growth is taking place rapidly in this region by the development of protoplasmic masses. My reasons for believing this are the following: A. The nuclei in the multinucleate cells are frequently seen crowded together as if they had developed in protoplasmic masses. B. In the protoplasmic masses the quantity of cytoplasm is out of proportion to the number of complete nuclei present. C. Developing nuclei have been actually observed in the cyto- plasm. The different stages of nuclear formation are shown by the following: 1 A similar process was suggested long ago by Schleiden and Schwann. 396 DALTON G. PAXMAN. (a) The chromidia, or diffused nucleus. (&) The irregular chromatin granules formed by the union of numerous chromidia and surrounded by a clear zone. (c) The nuclear membranes of the nuclei in the masses vary considerably from delicate, scarcely discernible membranes to heavy, well developed ones. D. These masses appear to arise by the simultaneous growth of cytoplasm and chromidial extrusions from the nuclei of certain cells. 3. The degenerate character of the nucleus is perhaps the result of the parasitic habit of the cestode. I wish here to express my sincere thanks to Dr. R. T. Young, whose valuable criticisms and suggestions made this work possible. I also wish to express my indebtedness to Dr. B. H. Ransom for identifying my material. LITERATURE. Child, C. M. '<>7a Studies on the Relation Between Amitosis and Mitosis. BIOL. BULL., Vol. XII., pp. 89-114. 'o7b Ibid., Vol. XII., pp. 175-224. '070 Ibid., Vol. XIII., pp. 138-160. 'O7d Ibid.. Vol. XIII., pp. 165-184. '10 Ibid., Vol. XVIII. , pp. 109-119. '01 Ibid., Vol. XXI., pp. 280-296. Harman, Mary T. '13 Method of Cell-Division in the Sex Cells of Taenia teniseformis. Journ. Morphol., Vol. XXIV., pp. 205-242. Richards, A. 'n The Method of Cell Division in the Development of the Female Sex Organs, of Moniezia. BIOL. BULL., Vol. XX., pp. 123-179. Young, R. T. '08 The Histogenesis of Cysticercus pisiformis. Zool. Jahrb. (Anat. und Ont.), Vol. XXVI., pp. 183-254. '10 The Somatic Nuclei of Certain Cestodes. Archiv. fur Zellforschung, pp. 140-164. '13 The Histogenesis of the Reproductive Organs of Taenia pisiformis. Zool. Jahrb. (Anat. und Ont.), Vol. XXXV., pp. 355-419. 398 DALTON G. PAXMAN. FIG. i. FIG. 2. FIG. 3. FIG. 4. FIG. 5. FIG. 6. present. FIG. 7. FIG. 8. nucleus, j. FIG. 9. EXPLANATION OF PLATE. Multinucleate cell, a. Nuclei with indistinct membranes, b and c. Nuclei, d and e, lacking nuclear membranes. Chromatin granules, /, in the cytoplasm. Large chromatin granule, g, in cytoplasm. A developing protoplasmic mass, h, in which no definite nucleus is A lobe, i, of a protoplasmic mass in which no definite nucleus is present. A large protoplasmic mass in the body region which contains only one Protoplasmic masses, k, developing in the body region. BIOLOGICAL BULLETIN, VOL. XXIX. PLATE I. --d Fiq.f, tf. 5 . D. G. PAXMAN I (TO LIBRARY