cea BODY AT WORK Digitized by the Internet Archive in 2007 with funding from Microsoft Corporation http://www.archive.org/details/bodyatworktreati0Ohilluoft = : . . . \ | ; 2 CORTEX OF THE THE UM. Fic. 1.—PHOTOMICROGRAPHS OF CELLS OF EBR » iv D CEI CEREBELLUM AN BEDS. see p For description wspiece. Front SE on. THE. PRINCIPLES | * OF PHYSIOLOGY a : | yon BY | ALEX’ HILL, M.A, M.D., F.R.CS. SOMETIME MASTER OF DOWNING COLLEGE, CAMBRIDGE WITH 46 ILLUSTRATIONS ales LONDON < EDWARD ARNOLD = es 1908 [All rights reserved] PREFACE Few subjects are as well provided with text-books as physi- ology; yet it may be doubted whether the interests of the amateur of science have been adequately cared for. From his point of view there are certain obvious drawbacks to even the most admirable of text-books. Writing for medical students, their authors assume that their readers have passed through two years of preliminary training in physics, chemistry, and biology ; they take for granted that they will have the privilege of supplementing their study of the theory of physi- ology with practical work in a laboratory ; they treat all parts of the subject with equal thoroughness. In this book I have endeavoured to describe the phenomena of life, and the principal conclusions which have been drawn as to their interdependence and as to their causes, in language which will be understood by persons unacquainted with the sciences upon which physiology is based. I have omitted all reference to experimental methods and to the technique of the science, save when a knowledge of the means by which information has been obtained is essential to a comprehension of its bearing. I have passed over such sections of the subject as are generally considered unsuit- able for ordinary discussion. And since this book neither aims at being an introduction to the systematic study of physiology, nor poses as an aid in the preparation for professional examina- tions, I have treated with some thoroughness the more recondite and the more suggestive results of recent research, and have tried to indicate the trend of modern thought regarding problems as yet unsolved. I have endeavoured to reflect the Vv vi PREFACE intrinsic interest of the science apart altogether from its medical applications. / An author who attempts the popular exposition of a science must stand sufficiently far away from his subject to lose sight of its details, whilst keeping its outlines clearly in view. The difficulty of finding such a position is probably greater in the case of physiology than in that of any other science. Few of its conclusions are indisputable—even those which seem to be most in accord with the balance of evidence. If my treatment of any vexed questions is unjustifiably dogmatic, this will, I trust, be attributed to the desire to present a definite picture, and not to forgetfulness of considerations which seem to call for qualified statements. All physiologists will agree that a book which recorded every piece of evidence which is difficult to reconcile with the views generally adopted would not only extend to an inordinate length, but would leave a very indefinite impression on the mind of the reader. In many cases the value of a conclusion depends upon the reputation for insight and accuracy of the physiologist who recorded the observations upon which it is based. It is no want of appreciation of the genius of the workers who have contributed most largely to the advance of the science which has led me to omit, save in a few classical instances, the names of all authorities. It is solely due to a desire to lighten this book of all details not essential to the comprehension of the proposi- tions which it sets forth. The illustrations are reproductions of blackboard drawings. A few of them have already appeared in my Physiologist’s Note- book and Primer of Physiology ; but the large majority are now printed for the first time. ALEX HILL. November, 1908. A ! fo CONTENTS PTER PAGE I. PROLEGOMENA - - - - - - 1 IL, THE BASIS OF LIFE - : : : 2 - 6 Ill. THE UNIT OF STRUCTURE~ - - - - - 26 | _‘IV. THE FLUIDS OF THE BODY - : ; : ae ; V. INTERNAL: SECRETIONS - - - - - 84 VI. DIGESTION” - - - - - - - 96 | VII. RESPIRATION - - - ae - - - 164 VIII. EXCRETION - : a} ; ; - 194 IX. THE CIRCULATION - - - - - eral X. MUSCLE - - = tke - - - 248 XI, THE NERVOUS SYSTEM - : - - - 293 XII, SMELL AND TASTE - - - - - - 364 XIII. VISION - - - - - - - 372 XIV. HEARING - - - - 256 - - 404 XV. SKIN-SENSATIONS - - - - - = ADS XVI. VOICE AND SPEECH - - - - - - 431 INDEX - : - - - - - 44] “U LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS r f & 2. FIG. PAGE 1. PHOTOMICROGRAPHS OF Bratn-TissuE - - - Frontispiece 2. THE ORGANS OF THE CHEST AND ABDOMEN - : - xi 3. Mucous GLAND, CAPILLARIES, AND CONNECTIVE-TISSUE Spaces - 38 4. BLoop-CorPuUscLEs - - - - - - 60 5. SPLEEN-PULP, WITH PHAGOCYTES - - - - - §] 6. DuoDENUM AND NEIGHBOURING ORGANS - - - - 99 7. A LoBuLE oF THE LiIvER AND LiveR-CELLS - - - 160 8. THe DIAPHRAGM DURING INSPIRATION AND EXPIRATION - - 172 9. THe KIDNEY - - - - - ‘ - 197 10. THe Heart In LONGITUDINAL SECTION” - - - - 218 11. Hortzontat SECTION OF THE HEART, SHOWING ITS VALVES - 9225 12. SECTION OF THE WALL oF A SMALL ARTERY - - - 233 13. KyMOGRAPH - - - - - - - - 238 14. SPHYGMOGRAPH - - - - - - - 244 15. BLoop-PREssuURE TRacInes - - : - - 945 16. Minute StrucTURE OF MuscLE-FIBRES~ - - - - 262 17. Toe Biceps MuscLtE In ACTION” - - - - - 286 18. ELectric ORGAN oF SKATE - 5 : . - 289 19. Minute Structure or A NERVE-FIBRE- - - - - 296 20. GANGLION-CELLS WITH NEURO-FIBRILLZ OF THE LEECH - - 298 21. Tot DEVELOPMENT OF THE GRANULES OF THE CEREBELLUM - 304 22. TigRoips AND NevuRo-FIBRILLE~ - - - - - $21 23. MInuTE STRUCTURE OF THE CORTEX OF THE CEREBELLUM - 339 24, MINUTE STRUCTURE OF THE CORTEX OF THE CEREBRUM - «. AT 25. FuNcTIONAL AREAS OF THE CORTEX OF THE CEREBRUM - - 352 26. TastTE-BuLBS - - - - - - - 368 27. HorizontaL SECTION OF THE EYE - - - = OTe 28. DEVELOPMENT OF THE CRYSTALLINE LENS - - - 374 29. Purxinsn’s SHADOWS OF THE VESSELS OF THE RETINA - - 375 30. RETINA ADAPTED FOR OBSCURITY AND FOR Bricut Licut = oll 31. SIMULTANEOUS CONTRAST - - : - - - 383 32. FORMATION OF AN IMAGE ON THE RETINA - - - 391 33. THe Form or THE EYEBALL IN SHORT Siaut, NoRMAL SIGHT, AND Lone Sicnt~ - - - so ee - - 392 ix ee. Tus Organ or Cort : | cern e: ES . 41. NERVES OF THE CORNEAL SELES) - : s A 42. ToucH-CoRPUSCLES - F . : : 4 43. PactnIaN CoRPUSCLE _~_- Ah pine : : z 44. Tur GLoTTIs - . ‘ g a 45. Tom Larynx IN Lonerrupina SECTION - Pay eS *, 46. Tur Larynx FROM THE RicuT SIDE Te i a Le : : - NOTE ON THE FRONTISPIECE Four photomicrographs of cells or parts of cells of brain-tissue, coloured by the chrome-silver method (cf. p. 293). . A. Cell of Purkinje from the cerebellum of a man aged 45. At the bottom of the photograph is seen the rounded cell-body, with the commencement of its axon. The summit of the cell-body bears an elaborately branched system of dendrites, spread out in the plane of the section. | B. A single basket-cell of the cortex of the cerebellum (very highly magnified). The oval cell-body gives origin to four dendritic pro- cesses which branch. Thorns are to be seen on the larger process which ascends on the right. From the same process, near its origin, springs a delicate axon which thickens as it proceeds to form a basket at the right-hand lower corner of the photograph. Two other branches of the same axon, which form baskets around other Purkinje-cells, are faintly visible, although out of focus. C. Seven or eight pyramids from the cortex cf the cerebrum of a hedgehog. A little below the centre of the photograph is seen a large pyramid with a single thorny apical process which bifurcates, several basal dendrites and an axon. In the upper part of the photograph are seen the apical processes of a number of pyramids of which the bodies were not included in the section. D. The margin of the cortex (subiculum cornu Ammonis) from the same specimen. A single row of pyramids extends across the photograph. They are remarkable for the richness of branching of their basal pro- cesses, which has earned for the cells which comprise this sheet the name of ‘‘ double pyramids.” All four sections were cut vertically to the surface. 435 nt F. 4 * Ly 1g o> “i 4 , f ‘ ent ee eed whe a, : a , ; 4 4 Pie" . ‘ < < | Sahoained i in 1 this process. nig a bird be fed poi a urea, c ore upon various salts of ammonia, its liver will change them into uric acid. Lactate of ammonia is the nitrogen-containing com- pound with which the liver has normally to deal. It can handle almost any other combination of nitrogen with equal ease. In the protoplasm of the liver the atoms in the molecule of lactate of ammonia are rearranged. The molecules are con- densed ; water is set free ; oxidation occurs. It seems almost as if molecules, when in contact with protoplasm, lose their individuality. Their atoms fall into new groups. Chains which the chemist finds so difficult to break—chains from which he can remove a link only by insinuating another and a stronger—are, when in contact with protoplasm, groups of isolated links. The links rearrange themselves. They join into new circlets, larger, smaller, more open, closer. As grains of sand on a metal plate group themselves in harmony with the vibrations caused in the plate by drawing a violin bow across it, so the atoms answer to the forces which set proto- plasm vibrating. There is no waste of force. The chemist may need to enclose sawdust and lime in a crucible heated in an electric furnace if he wishes to compel them to combine as carbide. He supplies energy enormously in excess of the amount which the new compound will lock up. Under the influence of protoplasm the reactions which occur are exactly proportional to the amount of energy supplied. Or, if it be a reaction by means of which energy is set free, it occurs spon- taneously. No energy is absorbed in setting it going. All the energy liberated is effective. The chemist very frequently needs to heat a substance in order to cause it to decompose, even though it be falling from a less stable to a more stable state. | Vital chemistry and mineral chemistry are so widely different in their methods that one is tempted to think of them as different in kind. We find it very difficult to look at both from the same point of view. Men’s minds are preoccupied with the things that they have to do for themselves. The chemistry of the laboratory is seen as a science circumscribed by the labora- tory walls. If it were possible to stand outside, it would be evident that it is only a part of the science of molecular change. Matter changes its state under the influence of force. Many a nts are effected by the chemist which do not o nature. He has an almost infinite range of action. Yet: many of the rearrangements of matter and force which are occurring in the dandelion on his window-sill (if the fumes of sulphuretted hydrogen have not killed it) he is unable to _ reproduce. It is largely a question of waste. Nature works B: with greater precision than the chemist ; but the chemist could - do all that Nature does if he had but the same control of force. We have spoken of the beaches which occur in protoplasm as _ divisible into two great series—the one ascending, constructive, endothermal; the other descending, destructive,exothermal. In _ the one series energy is locked up ; in the other series it is set free. Synthesis and analysis are names applied to the two series respectively. Synthesis is characteristic of plants, although analysis is also perpetually occurring. Plants fix carbon from the air and liberate oxygen. They also respire, setting free carbonic acid. Analysis is characteristic of animals, although synthesis is not excluded. Of the chemical processes which occur in plants very little is known. Few halting-places between raw materials and finished products can be marked. The final products are sugars and starches, oils, proteins, and a vast number of other substances—alkaloids, glucosides, etc. Condensation, de- hydration, and deoxidation are the methods by which the syn- thesis of these compounds is accomplished. These methods are adopted. simultaneously in varying degree. The large group of bodies known as sugars and starches are, with few exceptions, built on the C,H, model; in fruit sugar, C,H,,0O,, six atoms of carbon are linked to one another and to six molecules of water. The formula of starch is (C,H,,0;),. Not only has water been removed from the molecule, but an unknown number of molecules have been linked together. This conden- sation and’ dehydration is effected whenever sugar carried in cell-sap is deposited as starch in seeds or tubers. These com- pounds are hexatomic. The chemist pictures them as made by the union in the first place of six atoms. As small drops unite to form larger ones, so small molecules, under the direction of the protoplasm of plants, close together. The reactions which characterize animal protoplasm are of a different kind, They belong to the dcivcntiing series. Close oe molecules are unfolded. Water is incorporated with them. Hydrogen and carbon are oxidized into water and carbonic acid. - The conversion into sugar of glycogen or of starch may be taken as an illustration of expansion. Starch, (C,H,,0;),, becomes maltose, C,,.H,,0,,, and then dextrose, C,H,,0,. The grouped molecule of starch opens out. The breaking of the double molecule of maltose into two molecules of dextrose is a further illustration of progress towards simplicity. Hydra- tion, union with H,O, accompanies this expansion. Hydrolysis is the secret of almost all digestive acts. Starch is hydrolysed into sugar, fat hydrolysed into glycerin and fatty acid, proteins hydrolysed into peptones. All the chemical transformations which protoplasm is able to accomplish are of the nature of fermentations. The term fermentation was first applied to the effervescence which occurs in grape-juice when its sugar is being converted into alcohol, carbonic acid gas, and certain substances which appear in relatively.small quantities. It was discovered later that the yeast which effects this change is a unicellular plant. The term “fermentation ’’ was extended to the production of vinegar from alcohol, and eventually to all such reactions as are carried out by living organisms, or by the secretions or products of living organisms, without the destruction of the agent which is effective in the process. A ferment is an organic body which brings about changes in other bodies with- out itself undergoing change. At the end of the process, how- ever prolonged, there is as much ferment as there was at the beginning, and its chemical nature is the same. Rennin has been made to curdle nearly a million times its weight. of milk, pepsin to digest half a million times its weight of fibrin. As the ferment is not consumed, there is no relation, except one of speed, between the ferment and the quantity of fermentable substance which it is able to transform. We said that a fer- ment is an organic body. It is necessary to introduce the qualification “organic,” because certain reactions termed ‘* catalyses ”? which occur in mineral chemistry resemble fer- mentations in respect of the non-destruction of the agent which serves as intermediary. If a solution of cane-sugar containing a very small quantity of sulphuric acid is boiled, the cane-sugar _ ‘THE BASIS OF LIFE ete 3, “inverted.” _ It is changed into a mixture of fruit-sugar and : ‘The ferment invertin of the cee Juice and of 2 remains unchanged, so also the sulphuric acid is found in the - mixture unchanged in nature and in amount after an unlimited inversion of cane-sugar. Great stress was formerly laid upon _ the similarity between fermentation and catalysis. It has now been shown that catalytic actions are not necessarily of the same nature as fermentation, although the results and, as far as is visible, the means are similar. For example, finely divided platinum (or, better, palladium) causes an indefinite quantity of oxygen and hydrogen to unite. ‘The reaction comes within the category of catalyses. But it is widely different from a % fermentation. The metal causes hydrogen to condense, and actually absorbs it into its surface layer. In the liquid form hydrogen cannot resist combination with oxygen. This may be termed a “ physical phenomenon,” adopting the common distinction between chemistry and physics. There is no reason for thinking that fermentations can be explained in so simple a way. They may, however, be grouped under the designa- tion “‘ catalyses.”’ As the initial conditions and final results are similar, it is inevitable that fermentations and catalyses should obey the same “laws” as to mass action, speed, effect of accumulation of products of action, and the like ; but it does not follow that invertin and sulphuric acid produce their effects in the same way. Fermentations are instances of catalysis, but all catalytic actions are not fermentations. So far from dwelling upon the resemblance between fermen- tation and the catalysis of mineral chemistry, chemists now- adays incline to regard fermentation as essentially a reaction of life. It is very difficult, when attempting to present ideas which are new to thought, to adapt, without ambiguity, existing words. It would be absurd to talk of a substance removed from yeast or bacteria or blood-corpuscles by a process which involves cooling with liquid air, grinding with powdered glass, solution in water, precipitation with absolute alcohol, and re-solution in water, as alive. Yet, unlike any known mineral product, it is easily killed. Ferments are not destroyed by cold, but their activity is arrested. They are most active at about the body temperature. Their activity is annihilated 2 a _ ‘THE BODY AT WORK by heating them, in solution, to the temperature at which . albumin coagulates—a little over 50° C. Although they are — not alive, their behaviour very closely resembles that of living 4 matter. They can be obtained only from living things. They — produce their effects even though they are present in almost infinitely small quantity. It is impracticable to make a chemical analysis of a ferment, owing, in the first place, to the very small amount available for analysis, and, in the second place, be-— cause of the impossibility, with existing methods, of obtaining a ferment pure. The amount of ferment present in even a great mass of yeast, or in many pounds of salivary gland or pancreas, is extremely small. However prepared, it is always accom- panied with proteid substances. It is impossible to say whether ferments, like proteins, have heavy nitrogen-contain- ing molecules. The fact that they are not diffusible suggests that they have. It would be straining language to term fermentation a phe- nomenon of life ; worse, to define life as a sequence of fermen- tations. Yet it is safe to say that all the chemical changes carried out by living organisms are fermentations. Fermenta- tion and the chemistry of life are almost synonymous terms. A very large number of ferments are already known. Each has its own specific work to do: “To every fermentable sub- stance is fitted a ferment, as a key to a lock.”’ It will be under- stood, from what has been already said regarding our inability to determine the composition of any ferment, that we cannot say whether or not these various ferments differ one from another in chemical constitution. They are classified according to their action, and not according to their nature. Those which build up are termed “synaptases”’ (cvvdrtTw, I unite) ; those which decompose, or hydrolyse, “ diastases ” (Sedo Tracts, Separa- tion). The termination “ase ”’ is added to the name of the sub- stance upon which the ferment acts, except in cases in which other terms have already become so general as not to be dis- placeable: amylase, hydrolysing starch; sucrase, inverting cane- sugar; protease, hydrolysing proteins. Unfortunately, there - is little uniformity in this nomenclature ; amylopsin, invertin, pepsin, are terms used as often as those terminating in “ase.” As a distinguishing termination, “in ”’ or “sin ”’ is less desirable than “ ase,”’ owing to the fact that it has been appropriated e t Patiation of the names of albuminoids—e. g.; ‘chondrin, 1 mucin, e various ferments are substances which protoplasm sets ide for specific purposes. Primitively, contact with the \bstance to be fermented determined the nature of the ferment assigned to the task. There are reasons for thinking that proto- _ plasm still retains its power of making a suitable response ; _ cases may be cited in which the lock presented to protoplasm _ shapes the wards of the key. In such cases the fermentable _ substance provokes the formation of the ferment. But, for the _ most part, in situations where particular ferments are regu- larly needed, protoplasm has acquired the habit of making such ferments and no others. The cells of salivary glands accu- mulate ptyalin, the cells of gastric glands accumulate pepsin, during the intervals between meals. The capacity of protoplasm for producing a new ferment when it is needed is shown by such examples as the following : Blood-plasm contains a variety of proteid substances. If a solution of white of egg be added to it, the mixture is clear and uniform. Yet egg-albumin is treated by the blood as a foreign body, a poison. When injected into the veins of a living animal, some of it is excreted by the kidneys, some destroyed in the blood-stream. If several successive doses of egg-albumin are injected into an animal (it is most convenient to inject it into the peritoneal cavity), the power of the blood to destroy the intruder is greatly increased. If now a specimen of blood be taken, and the plasma or serum mixed with egg-albumin, the mixture is no longer clear. The egg- albumin is precipitated. The blood of the animal thus “ pre- pared ”’ has developed a ferment, termed a “ precipitin,’’ which throws down egg-albumin. Jf instead of egg-albumin, which, although a foreign body, is comparatively innocent, a substance _ which is distinctly poisonous, toxic, be injected into an animal, _ the first dose, if a large one, will prove fatal. If, however, the _ first dose be small, and succeeding doses progressively larger, the animal acquires the power of tolerating a quantity of the poison much larger than would have proved fatal in the first instance. A classical example of this, because it afforded an opportunity of directly observing under the microscope the ‘difference between ‘“‘ unprepared ” blood and blood from an 2—2 20 THE BODY AT WORK immune animal, is the acquisition by a mammal of the power — of tolerating the injection of the blood of an eel. Lel’s blood contains a toxin which destroys the red blood-corpuscles of a mammal. The dissolution of the blood-corpuscles may be watched with the microscope. If successively increasing doses of serum of eel’s blood be injected into the body of a rabbit, the rabbit acquires the power of resisting the toxin. Further than this, the serum of the immune rabbit injected into a rabbit which has not been prepared confers immunity upon the latter. If the blood of the prepared animal be mixed with the blood of an unprepared rabbit and with eel’s serum, and the mixture examined under the microscope, it will be seen that red blood-corpuscles are no longer dissolved. The immune serum is able to save the blood-corpuscles of the un- prepared blood from destruction. During its course of pre- paration the rabbit developed an antitoxin. If germs of diphtheria are injected into the blood of a horse, the first injections give rise to marked febrile symptoms. After a number of injections the horse becomes completely tolerant of the virus. Not only does its blood develop suffi- cient antitoxin to protect it against the toxin of diphtheria, however large may be the quantity injected into its system, but the serum of the prepared horse, when injected beneath the skin of a child suffering from diphtheria, carries with it sufficient antitoxin to destroy the toxin which has gained admission to the child’s blood. | Many more instances might be cited of this capacity of developing “‘ antibodies” of protoplasm. The leucocytes of the blood are incessantly adapting their chemistry to the needs of the economy. All the tissues, it may be supposed, possess the power of developing resistant ferments; but the leucocytes (Fig. 4) are the undifferentiated cells, the maids-of-all-work. They have not specialized as makers of ptyalin or makers of pepsin. They are not completely given up to lifting weights, like muscles, or carrying messages, like nerves. Bacteria are the world’s scavengers. To them ultimately belongs the task of reducing organic matter to the salts which plants reorganize. The cycle of life would be broken if bac- teria were suppressed. No sooner has an animal fallen than these little agents commence their beneficent task of resolving _ their work. They may steal portions of the derelict, use them for fuel, or patch them between their own ribs. But they, too, will soon lie breathless on the ground ; and the bacteria are always ready to finish their interrupted task. Why should they wait until the slight change occurs, important to us, but of little consequence to them, which marks the transition of living protoplasm into dead proteins? There is nothing in the constitution of protoplasm which makes it harder to break up than protein. There is no quality inherent in living matter which makes it resistant of decay. We resent the officiousness _ which prompts bacteria to obtain entrance into the ship while it is still under full sail, with a view to commencing the work of demolition. Deep in our minds lies the conviction that it is contrary to the rules of Nature. We are especially annoyed at the many ruses bacteria adopt to disguise their personalities. The bacteria of the soil we can keep at a proper distance. But bacteria of the stream, bacteria of milk, bacteria of the breath that would betray us with a kiss! It is hard to recognize that they are fairly and squarely playing their part. Birds and insects we can beat off with our hands. Our invisible enemies are everywhere. They are constantly insinuating themselves through scratches in the skin, through abrasions in the mouth, through surfaces of the intestine left unprotected owing to the desquamation of its epithelium. But if we are constantly open to attack, we are policed by myriads of zealous leucocytes, ever ready to reduce the invaders to impotence. The germs which have found entrance fire off a toxin. The leucocytes reply with an antitoxin. There is absolutely no limit to the power of protoplasm to protect itself, if only it be not taken by surprise. It can resist any organic poison if it is allowed a sufficient time to produce the antipoison. The ferment of pancreatic juice, trypsin, is a poison which is unlikely to find its way into the blood. When injected it produces disastrous results owing to its immense activity in digesting proteins. An animal “ prepared ”’ by the injection of successive doses of trypsin develops an antitrypsin. Injection of pancreatic juice no longer does it any harm. Tapeworms which live in the intestines;are bathed in pancreatic juice ; they are con- stantly exposed to its digestive action. They are not digested, if suisaceed from the ‘bodies of capew eee kad added to wae = creatic juice, renders it incapable of digesting albumin. The _ antikinase does not destroy trypsin, but destroys kinase, be ae 7 co-operation of which is essential to its activity. 5 Not only has protoplasm the power of meeting with an antiferment any ferment which might prove prejudicial to its — own integrity ; but after it has been once attacked it continues _ to defend the vulnerable spot. Its tactics are, it must be con- fessed, somewhat like those of the dusky warrior who, during his first lessons in the art of boxing, made a point of covering with his fist the place where he had just been hit ; but even its power of remembering its last injury is of supreme value to the human race. Before the age of sanitary science, and even, in certain backward communities, in these days of its beneficent rule, conditions producing disease were not necessarily set right as soon as the epidemic was over. The close-packed inhabitants of a ghetto were continuously exposed to germs of typhoid fever, small-pox, whooping-cough. But after their protoplasm had once responded to the need for the production of an antigerm, it either continued for many years to keep a stock in hand, or it kept the recipe within easy reach. The memory of protoplasm is amazing. It is commonly said that vaccination is an absolute protection for seven years. There is no doubt but that the immunity from small-pox which it induces, if gradually lessening, lasts for life. The disease, if it attacks a person who has been vaccinated in infancy, is relatively harmless. Inoculation, vaccination, is the boxing-master’s method of utilizing the self-protective instinct of the dusky warrior. Knowing that his pupil will for a long while continue to cover an injured spot, he asks himself: ‘“‘ Where is he most likely, when it comes to a serious contest, to be hit ?”? Then he gives him a gentle tap in that particular place. Does he need to know how to defend himself against small-pox ? Give him cow-pox. Is he likely to receive a knock-down blow from typhoid fever? Just show him what it feels like to have a gentle shake. Educate his protoplasm to make antityphoid = giving him the ie tepioid germ in ack an attenuated it oarietat do him any harm. chemistry of protoplasm is a science which is growing = _ rapidly, or, to speak less arrogantly and more correctly, our _ knowledge of the ways of protoplasm, the Chemist, has greatly _ increased during the last few years. We can but watch proto- _ plasm at work. Our experiments, so called, are but windows which we open in the walls of his laboratory. We cannot take the work out of his hands. The methods of mineral chemistry are useless in this search for knowledge. And, naturally, the longer we watch, the more details do we discover in what seemed at first a generalized procedure. We recognize that several manipulations are required in the carrying out of a reaction which hitherto we believed to take place in a single stage. This is not the place in which to give an account of a subject re- garded as belonging, owing to its applications, to the province of pathology. But Nature is one, however many be the com- panies into which we divide the explorers of her secrets. We have attempted the merest outline of the observations made up to the present, and have submitted the results for the sake of the light which they throw upon the way in which ferments are prepared as they are wanted to meet the needs of normal everyday digestion and metabolism, rather than for the purpose of showing the methods by which protoplasm combats disease. Amongst the chemical phenomena of life is respiration. Respiration in this very general sense means oxidation. The force which is exhibited in living is obtained from the union of organic materials with oxygen under the direction of protoplasm. This is true of plants as well as of animals. It is true even of the subdivision of bacteria, termed anaerobic, because they cannot live in air. They secrete ferments which enable them to decompose compounds which contain oxygen, in order that they may use the oxygen for respiration. It might have been supposed that green plants which are receiving radiant energy from the sun would convert this energy into the forces which enable protoplasm to display the phenomena of life. But this is not so. The energy which green plants obtain from the sun is used in constructive metabolism, and not in maintaining life. Life-force, if we may use the expression, is derived from the oxidation 7.2). a. ae) % ee hatin’ nae, oo Lae — 24 THE BODY AT WORK. of the substances which the sun’s rays enable the plant to make. ~ aph in the sp Be ideeon ie Che Lv did t be oe in the same way. There is undue pr on the vessels in which the blood circulates hrougl cee inflamed pleura (the investing membrane of the — s and lining membrane of the chest), yet the walls of the capillaries fail to maintain a proper balance between blood _ and lymph. Hitherto we have spoken of the lymphatic system as a 4 labyrinth of communicating spaces containing stagnant fluid, _ which is kept in a fitting state by egress and ingress out of and into blood. Such a mental picture is substantially correct. But the system is complicated by the presence of lymphatic vessels. Cells of the connective-tissue sponge-work arrange themselves side by side. They flatten into endothelial scales. The borders of the scales close up. They form lymphatic channels, wider than blood-capillaries, but strictly comparable in every other respect. The lymph capillaries unite into larger vessels. The larger vessels are connected by cross- branches ; they form plexuses. Their walls are strengthened with fibrous tissue. Like the veins, they are abundantly provided with valves, which check any tendency to a backward flow on the part of the fluid which they contain. Lymphatic plexuses surround and accompany the larger bloodvessels. They are disposed on the surface of muscles and glandular tissues. They are abundant beneath the skin. Nearly three centuries ago the lymphatic vessels of the mesentery, which collect products of digestion, especially fat, from the walls of the alimentary canal, were recognized owing to the milkiness of their contents after a meal. They were, on this account, termed “lacteals.”” Other lymphatic vessels, owing to their transparent walls and colourless contents, are not easily seen ; _ but they are readily injected with mercury or other fluids _ which render them conspicuous. In the upper part of the thigh, in the armpit, or in the neck, they are about large enough to admit a crow-quill. Those from the lower limbs, from the viscera, and from the walls of the abdomen converge to a receptacle which lies in front of the spinal column. The _ receptaculum chyli is continued upwards as the thoracic duct, which pours the lymph into the great veins of the 44 ‘THE BODY AT WORK left side of the neck and of the left arm just where they join together. The thoracic duct provides for the overflow of lymph from the spaces of the body. There is no circulation of lymph. Lymph from the liver and from the intestines is constantly draining into the thoracic duct, and thus returning to the blood-stream by a short direct route, entering it without the necessity for reabsorption through the walls of capillary vessels. By no means all of this fluid has exuded from the blood-stream. Much of it is water which was poured into the stomach as gastric juice, and into the intestines as the secre- tions of the pancreas and other glands, or imbibed through the mouth and absorbed by the lymphatics of the alimentary canal. The remainder of the water taken up from the ali- mentary canal enters its bloodvessels. The diluted blood flows to the liver, loaded with digested products which the liver will store. As the blood parts with them the additional water which has served for their transport exudes from the capillaries of the liver into lymphatics, which empty it into the thoracic duct. Large quantities of water are used in washing out digested products. Secreted into the alimentary canal by the digestive glands, it passes out through its wall as the vehicle of digested products. Collected by lymphatic vessels, it is either carried directly into the thoracic duct, or passed from lymph into blood, carried by blood to the liver, again transferred from blood to lymph, and borne by the lymphatic vessels of the liver to the thoracic duct. Water exuded from blood into lymph may be reabsorbed into the blood near the place where it was poured out, or it may reach the blood via the thoracic duct. It would seem that the former is the natural, the latter the emergency route ; the former the course taken when an organ is tranquil, the latter a necessity when the organ is active. If the large lymphatic vessels of a limb are cut, no lymph escapes from them so long as the limb is at rest. When the muscles contract lymph begins to flow. If the limb is flexed and extended by hand, lymph flows. If the muscles are squeezed or massaged, lymph flows. As the flow is set up both by active contraction of the muscles and by passive movements in which the muscles do not take part, it clearly must be due to external pressure yh: t yossals, ras pig are ioe, with valves, 1e¢ h aevors them into pumps. The fluid which Peactecn is bound to go forwards. Additional fluid is squeezed into them from the tissue-spaces. To a large extent, ‘s i solore, the outflow of lymph from contracting muscles is to be explained as the result of the pressure which the swelling ~ muscles exert upon the lymphatic vessels within their sheaths. But there is another factor which must not be overlooked, although it cannot readily be estimated. When a muscle is actively contracting its bloodvessels dilate. There is a greater exudation of lymph; and reabsorption by blood is not equal to the exudation. The surplus leaves the limb by the lymphatic vessels. A gland is never at rest. In the intervals between the ejection of its secretion its cells are preparing materials for the next outflow. Lymph is always flowing from a gland ; its amount increases as the activity of the gland increases. More lymph leaves the blood when the gland is exceptionally active than when it is relatively quiet. Some of it is not reabsorbed into the blood. A certain pro- portion of the waste products of the active gland are hurried away by the overflow system in the direction of the thoracic duct. Lymph is the reservoir of nutriment upon which every cell in the body draws. It is improbable that in health and under normal conditions the activity of any organ is ever restricted for want of sufficient food. As food is removed from lymph, it is instantly replaced by fresh food from the blood. There is some evidence—not very clear—that the removal of waste products offers greater difficulty than the renewal of supplies of food. When the activity of muscles has been excessively prolonged they ache. It has been supposed that their un- willingness to do more work is due, not to the exhaustion of the food which they use up when contracting, but to the inadequacy of the lymph and blood to carry off all refuse. This, at least, is the explanation of fatigue which is usually offered, although it is difficult to understand why the arrange- ments for removing waste products which have worked to perfection for eight hours should during the ninth hour become rapidly ineffective. Ii a frog’s muscle, cut out of the body, has been made: to | te it can ds is to wash away waste tia suet But this experiment. upon a tired, isolated muscle does not necessarily throw light upon the nature of fatigue i in muscles under normal — x , conditions. The isolated muscle is using up, in contracting, — food which it has stored. Cut off from the circulation, it has no means of getting rid of the lactic acid and other products into which food is changed. They may well have accumulated to a poisonous extent long before all the food has been used up. Hardly more cogent is the argument based upon the benefit which a tired man experiences from hot baths, massage, and the like. They take away the feeling of tiredness, but it does not follow that this result is due to the removal of waste products. Quickening the circulation of blood brings about renewal of the lymph. Renewal of lymph means fresh supplies of food as well as removal of waste products. Even human muscles are not perfect as machines. ‘They will not work for an unlimited spell. There comes a time when they must have rest. Something goes wrong in the admirable adjustment which has hitherto provided exactly the right amount of food and exactly the necessary freedom from the products of action. A feeling of fatigue is the signal that the apparatus is not in a condition to work longer; but whether this feeling is due to a dislocation of the balance of supply and loss, or to some deterioration of the apparatus which calls for rest and renova- tion, it is at present impossible to say. It is not due to the exhaustion of muscle food. A more powerful stimulus, the urgency of fright or some other strong emotion, or an electric current applied directly to the muscle or its nerve, will still induce vigorous contraction. The muscles of a hare that has been coursed until it can run no farther still contain glycogen, muscle-food. Glycogen is stored in the liver. Fat, if it is assimilated in excess of the needs of the body, accumulates in the connective tissues. Proteins, if in excess, are either destroyed by oxida- tion, or partly destroyed and partly converted into fat. In- creasing the amount and richness of the food does not, if nutrition is already at its best, improve the quality of the ny — is gine ne 3 Its petite is not im- 1g more food than enough. A perfect balance E iain as Sirtich nutriment as it needs. It cannot get more, Tt cannot lay by food and shirk work. If it did it would grow. d _ Reaching its optimum size, it would divide. Additional tissue _ would be formed. But when it does more work it needs more _ food ; and it is a matter of common experience that the system _ is soadjusted that food is supplied to the tissues, not reluctantly, but with a slight tendency towards generosity. Working harder than usual, they find the lymph by which they are _ bathed somewhat richer in the materials that they need than the necessities of the case demand. They are able not merely to obtain all they want, but a little more. Activity aia growth. Many attempts have been made to show that if a part of the body has more than its share of food it grows to an excessive size. John Hunter grafted a cock’s spur into its comb. It grew to monstrous dimensions. Such a result favours the view, but it is not quite conclusive. Undoubtedly the comb was richly supplied with blood, but it does not follow that the cells of the spur were able in their new situation to take advantage of this supply. Besides, the spur when projecting from the head was not subject to the accidents to which it was exposed whilst on the leg. Its size was not kept down by friction. Nor was it as hard and compact as it would have been in its normal situation. It is scarcely possible to devise any experiment that would be satisfactory now that the relations between blood and lymph and lymph and tissues are understood. In certain pathological conditions, however, hypertrophy is the result of the hyperemia of chroni¢ inflammation ; and there is little doubt that, if we could arrange for a certain group of cells to _ receive lymph richer in food and freer from waste products than the perfect adjustment of supply to needs normally allows, the cells would grow. _ Under perfectly healthy normal conditions growth can be _ induced only by use. Nature supplies the fuel which is used - during activity, and a balance of food available for the con- struction of additional machinery. The muscle which is 48 THE BODY AT WORK called upon to do work develops a greater capaale me a work. When nutrition is not at its best, the growth of muscle may be favoured by external pressure which squeezes lymph out of its tissue-spaces, and therefore leads to increased exudation from the blood. It is not improbable that in badly nourished tissues the circulation of blood is somewhat torpid and the lymph stagnant. A feeble circulation usually results in some cdema. ‘The muscles, or rather the connective tissue which envelops and penetrates them, feels doughy, instead of being, as it should be, firm and elastic. Under these conditions massage is undoubtedly of service. Squeezing the muscles displaces lymph, and, if the pressure is properly directed, drives it along the lymphatic vessels. Fresh lymph exudes from the capillary bloodvessels, and the muscle-fibres, surrounded with a more abundant supply of nutriment, benefit, as, in a vigorous person, they benefit from use. Lymph is an exudate from blood. Its composition therefore depends upon that of blood-plasm, but it tends to differ from it owing to the influence of two causes. In the first place, the walls of the capillary bloodvessels restrict exudation. Red blood-corpuscles cannot pass through them. Proteins which are non-diffusible are, according to the circumstances of the tissues, held back to a greater or to a less extent. ‘The pseudo- capillaries of the liver let them pass, as has already been said. The capillaries of the limbs restrict their passage to such pro- portions as, it may be supposed, are absolutely necessary for the nutrition of the tissues. In the second place, tissues remove food from lymph and add to it waste products. Hence the lymph issuing from a limb, after full contact with the tissues, contains less of the former and more of the latter—less sugar, for example, and rather more oxidized nitrogenous substances, lecithin and other things termed collectively “ extractives,” because they can be extracted from dried blood or lymph by ether. The reaction of lymph is alkaline. After a time it coagulates, but coagulation is slower, and the clot less firm than in the case of blood. As the composition of lymph depends upon the source from which, and the conditions under which, it has been obtained, it is unnecessary to state the results of a chemical analysis. ‘THE FLUIDS OF THE BODY y It suffices to say that lymph contains all the substances which are present in the plasma of blood, but not necessarily in the - same total amount or in the same relative proportions. Speak- ing generally, leucocytes are present in about the same numbers as in blood—6,000 to 8,000 to the cubic centimetre; but leucocytes are everywhere present: in blood, in the lymph, - in lymph-vessels, in the tissue-spaces. As they are not _ passively floating bodies like red blood-corpuscles, but active _ migratory organisms, they tend to accumulate in one situation and withdraw from another, in accordance with the opportuni- ties which the different localities afford. They desert effused lymph, blisters, ascitic fluid, and the like. They are not found in the lymph in the pericardium. There are fewer in the lymph coming from the intestines after a meal than in the same lymph during the intervals between meals. Their departure from effused lymph might easily be explained. It is not so easy to account for their comparative absence from the lymph in the lacteals when it is heavily charged with fat and other products of digestion. Such leucocytes as are present at this time are loaded with fat granules which they have stolen from the chyle, as the lymph in the lacteals is usually termed. One would need to be very intimate with a leucocyte before one ventured to give reasons for all its movements. Lymph contains the same proteid substances as blood, and in the same relative proportions, but usually in smaller quantity. : Incidental reference has been made to the great lymph-spaces —peritoneal, pleural, and pericardial. The brain and spinal cord are separated from their outer membranes by a lymph- space. There are also spaces within the brain—the ventricles —and a central canal in the spinal cord. The aqueous and vitreous humours of the eye are also lymph-spaces, although the latter contains some remnants of tissue. The joint cavities are lymph-spaces. So also are the burse which surround tendons or separate them from bones. It is not, however, justifiable to include all these cavities in a single category, either from the point of view of their purpose, their mode of formation, or the nature of their contents. The peritoneal, pleural, and pericardial spaces are parts of the great primitive body-cavity, or ccelom. The two first are potential rather than actual. Normally they contain just sufficient fluid to moisten 4 50 | THE BODY AT WO . nic the apposed surfaces of the endothelium which lines their walls and covers the organs which they contain. There is no fluid in them which can be collected and labelled “ peri- toneal”’ or “‘ pleural’’ fluid. The purpose of the spaces is to allow of movement without friction—in the one case of the intestines, in the other of the lungs. It is possible to take a spoonful or so of fluid out of the space which surrounds the heart. It has the usual composition of lymph. It contains proteins, but is not spontaneously coagulable. Leucocytes are absent, a fact which probably accounts for its not clotting. The fluid inside the cerebro-spinal system is extremely dilute. Its principal salt—its principal constituent, indeed—is sodic chloride. It contains hardly a trace of proteins, and these in a modified condition—proteoses. It also contains pyro- catechin, a benzoic alcohol. This substance has long been recognized as a constituent of cerebro-spinal fluid, owing to the fact that, like sugar, it reduces copper salts when heated with them in an alkaline solution. It appears to be one of the products of proteid decomposition. Although exuded as lymph from the bloodvessels of the chorioid plexuses, the composition of cerebro-spinal fluid has been profoundly changed by the activity—it might almost be called the digestive activity— of the epithelium which lines the cerebro-spinal canal. There is a theory that the ancestors of all vertebrate animals were organized on a very different plan from that of their distant descendants. Our cerebro-spinal canal was their stomach and intestine. It would appear that the lining epithelium of these organs, although disused for millions of years, cannot resist the temptation to digest the lymph which they contain ! The fluid in joints contains mucin (the essential constituent of mucus), or a substance resembling mucin. In this case the joint-membrane has added something to lymph without removing or destroying any of its other constituents. Other illustrations might be given showing how the plasma of blood is altered in composition while it is passing out of, or after it has passed out of, capillary bloodvessels. Per- haps it would be more logical to start on the outer side of the walls of the capillaries ; since blood may, very properly, be regarded as a tissue, dependent, like all other tissues, upon diffusion from lymph for the nutrient materials that it needs. : he w Il of th e alimentary ‘las it receives seeps via the np 1. mt ive them in the liver, its garde-~manger, to pick them up again as they are wanted. The torrent of lymph aa wh ich the thoracic duct discharges into the veins of the neck > "conveys the fat which could not traverse the walls of the capillary bloodvessels, and much of the reserve of food which the blood had deposited in the liver. Only about one-quarter of the fluid of the body (one-thirteenth of the body-weight) is included within the blood-system; but this enclosed fluid, owing to the fact that it is kept in circulation by the heart, replenishes and purifies the much larger quantity which does not circulate. The unenclosed lymph has in particular situa- tions a chemical composition which varies widely from that of the blood. Imagine a marsh through which a river flows— the vast plains of water-plants on the Nile above Fashoda, for example. There is a constant interchange between the flowing water of the river and the stagnant water of the marsh. In any given part of the marsh the quality of the water will depend upon what it has been able to take from, and what it — has given back to, the river ; upon what the water-plants have taken from it, and what they have added to it. Boats which cannot penetrate the walls of reed keep to the open channel of the Nile. Fish swim, now in the river, now in the narrow passages and open pools of the marsh. So it is, in a way, with the fluid in the spaces and cavities of the lymphatic system and in the bloodvessels which traverse them, and with its migratory inhabitants. In our extravagant analogy read leucocytes for fish. Fish have two reasons for wandering from river to marsh. Amongst the water-weeds they hunt for food ; they seek quiet places in which to breed. In this matter the analogy holds good. , A leucocyte may be overtaken with cell division anywhere—in the blood-stream or in a lymph- vessel. But cell division very rarely occurs except in certain favoured spots. The breeding-places chosen by leucocytes are sheltered situations in connective tissue where the blood- supply is abundant, and the eligibility of such a spot is much increased by its being near to a field where their services are likely to be called for. The nests of connective tissue made by the leucocytes are of three kinds, termed respectively diffuse adenoid tissue, lymph-follicles, and lymphatic glands. The : 4—2 a EN SS BY eee ae 723 4 ii fc ee Pe ee i 52 THE BODY AT WORK connective tissue beneath the mucous membrane of the whole of the respiratory tract—trachea, bronchi, and bronchioles— is diffuse adenoid tissue. It presents no special structure, but its spaces are packed with leucocytes in various stages of cell division, and young leucocytes, or lymphocytes, as they are usually named. Some of the lymphocytes make their way into the blood or into the lymph. Others, acquiring their full _ dimensions, scour the epithelium which lines the respiratory tract for germs and other foreign bodies which are drawn into the tract with inspired air. They may be seen pushing aside the cells of the lower strata of the epithelium, on their way to the surface, or returning to the subepithelial connective tissue with germs, or particles of soot, or débris of epithelial cells which they have taken into their substance (Fig. 4, B). The tonsils are examples of follicular lymphoid structures. They lie one on either side of the entrance to the gullet, between the two folds (the anterior and posterior pillars of the fauces) by which the soft palate is continued to the side of the tongue. Normally the tonsil is not visible, but when inflamed it may project sufficiently to be seen ; and its surface may then be covered with mucus and pus. It is liable to become enlarged in childhood, owing to chronic inflammation. A section of the tonsil shows it to consist of clusters of lymph- follicles lying beneath the mucous membrane. The term ** follicle ’’ is unfortunate. It conveys no idea of the form or structure of one of these masses of lymph-cells ; and it is, besides, applied to things of an entirely different character— for example, the pits of mucous membrane which sink down between the masses of lymphoid tissue in the tonsil. The expression “follicular tonsillitis ’’ does not refer to the lymph- follicles, but to the epithelial pits. It is a condition in which a drop of pus is to be seen in the mouth of each of the pits. A lymph-follicle is a small rounded clump of connective tissue, denser on its periphery than in its centre. Its bloodvessels are disposed chiefly on the periphery. Lymphatic streamlets arise in the centre. Its outer portion is closely packed with dividing lymph-cells and young leucocytes, which as fast as they are formed migrate towards the centre, and eventually escape from the follicle by the lymphatic vessels. The con- nective tissue which invests and separates the follicles is full _- 'THE FLUIDS OF THE BODY 53 of leucocytes. Removal of the tonsils is followed by no ill effects. They are not essential to our well-being. Neverthe- less, they have important functions to perform. They are barracks crowded with leucocytes, which guard the pass into the alimentary canal. Their leucocytes incessantly patrol the mucous membrane, capturing germs, removing fragments of injured epithelium, striving to make good the mischief to which this part of the alimentary canal is peculiarly liable. The enlargement of the tonsil which results from frequent sore throat is a response to the demand for an increase in the supply of these little scavengers, in order that they may cope, not only with objectionable things outside the walls, but with the still more pernicious germs which during an attack of sore throat succeed in breaking through the epithelium. It is the invaders which elude the vigilance of the leucocytes that cause fever and other general symptoms. Other notable groups of lymph-follicles are found in the middle portion of the small intestine, where they form oval patches, about three-quarters of an inch long by half an inch broad—Peyer’s patches. The leucocytes which are developed in them search the walls of the intestine for germs. During an attack of enteric fever the patches become inflamed, and one of the greatest risks which the patient runs is the risk of ulceration of a patch and the perforation of the intestinal wall. The abundant provision for the multiplication of leucocytes shows that the destruction of these cells must occur on an equally large scale. Every day large numbers die. Where this occurs, and how their dead bodies are removed, is not certainly known. Doubtless they are eaten by their fellows, their sub- ‘stance oxidized, and the products—carbonic acid, water, and nitrogenous waste—thrown into the lymph. There is some reason for thinking that a part of the nitrogenous waste is excreted in the form of uric acid (cf. p. 216). The daily pro- duction, and consequent destruction, of leucocytes shows that their metabolism is a factor which cannot be overlooked when we are making up the body’s accounts. The fixed tissues receive their nutriment in a digested con- dition. Leucocytes digest it for themselves. In many cases, although not in all, the cells of fixed tissues last throughout life, so far as their outer form is concerned, although their Se Pe eed BS Uy rea? Sea BA THE BODY AT WORK molecules are oxidized and replaced by new material. It is not improbable, therefore, that there is a difference between the metabolism of the fixed tissues and the metabolism of leucocytes. The whole of a wandering cell, its nucleus included, breaks down and has to be removed. We do not know that this occurs in the case of a fixed cell. On the strength of evidence which points, apparently, to a chemical relationship between nuclear substances and uric acid, it has” been inferred that the two chief nitrogenous products which are excreted by the kidney are divisible into the one which in the main represents the oxidation of fixed cells, urea, and. the other, uric acid, largely derived from the oxidation of wandering cells. The valiant leucocytes do their best to cope with all the rubbish, whether living or dead, that needs removal. They flock to any situation in which germs are numerous or tissue has been destroyed. If all goes well they take the foreign matter into their substance—dead tissue is matter foreign to the body—and either digest it in the course of their ordinary progress, or retreat with it, if they cannot digest it, to the nearest lymphatic gland. But in their efforts to reach objec- tionable matter they are apt to wander too far from the healthy lymph from which they obtain oxygen for their own respiration. Unable to breathe, they die. They lose the power of extruding pseudopodia. Their extensible, prehensile processes are drawn in. Assuming a globular form, they float helplessly in what once was lymph. Their body-proteins are largely changed to fat. As “ pus cells,” they are thrown off in the discharge from an ulcer, or accumulate in the cavity of an abscess. A pus cell is a dead and fattily degenerated leucocyte. The third kind of breeding-place of leucocytes, a lymphatic gland, has a more elaborate structure than the tissues with which we have already dealt. Lymphatic glands are about the size of beans, and of the same shape. They are found in the course of lymphatic vessels in situations where they are not exposed to pressure, such as the back of the knee, the groin, the front of the elbow, the armpit, in the neck above the collar-bone, and on either side of the sterno-mastoid muscle, behind the angle of the jaw. There are a number in the lom Kandi in the tienes: ‘Each ‘acetic gland i is invested _ by a strong fibrous capsule. Its artery enters, and its vein and efferent lymphatics leave, the concave side (the hilus) of gs ‘the gland. The lymphatic vessels which bring lymph to it pierce the capsule on its convex side. It is divisible into two parts : (1) The adenoid tissue which surrounds the artery and its branches ; (2) the open network of “ lymph-ways ” which invest this adenoid tissue. Leucocytes divide in the adenoid tissue. The young lymphocytes drop out into the lymph-ways. As a stream of lymph, brought by the afferent vessels, is always flowing into the lymph-ways, and out by the efferent vessel or vessels, the lymphocytes are carried with it towards the thoracic duct. A lymphatic gland is therefore an organ for adding leucocytes to lymph in the course of the lymph-stream. It has, however, another and equally important function. Leucocytes which have picked up germs or other foreign matter pass on with the lymph to a lymphatic gland. After entering its lymph-ways they leave the lymph-stream, squeeze into the adenoid tissue of the gland, and there come to rest with their burden. They remain in the gland until the foreign matter is digested, or, if it be indigestible, until they undergo dissolution, when the particles of soot or pigment are deposited from their débris in a harmless state. When the skin is tattooed, much of the Indian ink and other pigment remains where it was inserted with the needle, but some of it is picked up by leucocytes and carried to the nearest lymphatic gland. Lymphatic glands are barriers which stop the spread of infection. They are the stations to which our police carry captured germs. The skin of the heel is abraded. Germs from the soil, or elsewhere, which have accumulated in a dirty stocking—owing to the warm moisture enclosed by an imper- vious boot, the woollen’ covering of the foot is a peculiarly healthy place for germs—enter the opened lymph-spaces of the subcutaneous tissues. Leucocytes hasten to the spot. They seize the invaders with their pseudopodia, engulf them in their body-substance, enter lymphatic vessels, and are rolled away by the lymph-stream. The instinct which brings them in ever-increasing numbers to the breach in the protecting skin can be explained only in terms of force. From our own conscious action to the causes which determine the movements rn > é a “a Wa “ » 7 } al ; ear. a er ew CO ——" , - e e Saeee Se tdwten eek oe pe: a. % ie aF- le + we “aay 8 eo ee pie ae § 7 ‘ as - . > 56 THE BODY AT WORK of a leucocyte, or of an ameeba, is so deep a drop that we prefer to recognize in the latter a merely chemical attractive force. “‘ Chemiotaxis ” we term the influence which draws leucocytes to the place where food is abundant ; although it is also the place, one must admit, where in the interests of the body as a whole they run great risk of asphyxiation. It is appetite which draws a schoolboy to a bun-shop; a sense of duty prompts a fireman to risk his life in a chamber filled with smoke. We have no desire to humanize a leucocyte ; but it is difficult to emphasize too strongly its independence. It would be absurd to use terms which imply that a leucocyte has a self-directive power; yet it is equally misleading to describe its migration to the seat of injury, its retreat with ingested germs to a lymphatic gland, its wriggling from the lymph-ways of the gland into the shelter of its adenoid tissue, in terms which imply that the forces which direct it are known, and their mode of action understood. The success which attends the inroads of germs is due to their amazing capacity for multiplication when they reach lymph or blood. It is useless to attempt to form an idea of the rapidity with which they divide, since we have no data upon which to base cal- culations. If the leucocytes fail to deal with the first few that enter, germs soon swarm within the lymph-vessels. This leads to an inflammation of the walls of the vessels, which may then be seen as red lines beneath the skin. These red lines lead upwards towards the nearest lymphatic gland. The glands in the space behind the knee are not usually affected when the focus of infection is in the foot. The red lines can be traced up the inner side of the knee and the front and inner side of the thigh to the groin. The glands in this situation swell until they can be easily felt. If the mischief is in the hand, the gland at the elbow may be affected, but most of the lymphatics pass by it on their course to the glands in the armpit. If a sore throat is the source of infection, the glands beneath the angle of the jaw enlarge. Thus various glands block the further progress of infection. In doing this their resources may be strained to the uttermost ; they may enlarge, become tender, grow soft, fill with pus, break down and dis- charge the pus without the aid of a surgeon’s knife, although as soon as pus is recognizable within them it is wise to let it ss THE FLUIDS OF THE BODY 57 out. If germs pass through these first stations into the lymph- vessels beyond them, abscesses are formed in other situations. _~ A condition of ‘‘ blood-poisoning,” so called, is set up. The readiness with which leucocytes sacrifice themselves in _ their efforts to remove germs and decaying tissue is a matter of almost every-day experience. The fatty matter produced in the sebaceous glands of the skin normally overflows on to _ the surface. It serves to render the skin supple and imper- vious to water. Germs get into one of the sebaceous glands of the face or of the eyelid. The contents of the gland begin to decompose. Leucocytes enter it for the purpose of re- moving the putrescent substance. They lose their vitality and turn into pus corpuscles. The pimple or the stye bursts, and pus and fatty matter are discharged together. That the conversion of leucocytes into pus cells is due to want of oxygen has been shown by the following experiment : A minute piece of phosphorus is placed beneath the skin. Leucocytes gather round the spot with a view to removing the tissue which the phosphorus has destroyed. But phosphorus has so strong an affinity for oxygen that it exhausts the supply in the area of tissue which surrounds it. The leucocytes die before reaching the tissue immediately adjacent to the piece of phosphorus. Their dead bodies form round it a raised ring of pus cells. We can explain this readiness of leucocytes to sacrifice themselves in their efforts to reach foreign matter which needs to be removed, only by saying that the attraction of the food is greater than the repulsion of lymph destitute of oxygen. An amceba placed in comparable circumstances gives up the quest of food, however strongly chemiotaxic, and retreats towards water which contains oxygen sufficient to provide for its respiratory needs. Blood.—A portion of the body fluid is enclosed within vessels and kept in circulation by the heart. The heart pumps blood into the aorta. This trunk gives off large arteries, which in turn divide until the finest capillary vessels are reached. The capillary tubes reunite to form veins, which, with the excep- tion of those which collect food from the digestive organs, convey the blood right back to the heart. The veins which drain the stomach and intestines (the organs in which food is prepared for absorption) and the spleen (the organ in which Ae eae vessels of the liver reunite to fen the hopale ge _ which add the blood that has passed through that organ to | the-rest of the blood which is passing up the inferior vena a cava to the heart. A second capillary circulation is found B in the kidney also. The heart is four-chambered (Fig. 10). Its left ventricle a drives the blood round the systemic or greater circulation, the blood returning to the right auricle. The right ventricle drives the blood through the lesser or pulmonary circulation, from which it returns to the left auricle. The walls of all bloodvessels, except capillary tubes, are sufficiently thick to prevent the escape of any of the constituents of blood. To support the pressure of the blood which they contain, the arteries and the larger veins need walls of considerable thick- ness. The walls of the capillaries allow an interchange between blood and lymph in the manner already described (cf. p. 39). Blood fresh from the lungs, whether still in the pulmonary veins or in the systemic arteries, is scarlet in colour. Venous blood is darker and purple-red, the depth of its tint varying with the extent to which it has parted with its oxygen. It looks less opaque than arterial blood. With this exception, the physical properties and chemical composition of blood are remarkably constant in all parts of the body. Arterial blood contains more oxygen, venous blood more carbonic acid. Other chemical differences can be recognized, but they are relatively very small. The constancy in the constitution of blood is its most notable character. Bleeding, unless exces- sive, does not greatly affect it. The number of corpuscles is of course diminished, but even these are replaced with great rapidity. The plasma, after bleeding, soon recovers its proteins and salts. A similar readjustment occurs if normal saline solution (water containing 0-9 per cent. sodic chloride), or even a strong solution of salt, is injected into the blood. Within certain limits it is very difficult to disturb the balance of its constituents. It gets rid of substances added in excess, or replaces substances removed, with remarkable facility. If sugar (glucose) be injected into a vein, it escapes through the capillary walls into the lymph. After a short interval the i et ae ee is MO ae dae blood. nag an excess of gh a kind foreign to the blood or its own serum-albumin be injected, it is removed by the kidneys. The blood has various sources from which it can draw out "reserves of anything that is lacking, and various ways of _ getting rid of anything that is in excess. It draws upon the iy oe al in the tissue-spaces for water. It discharges salts into the lymph. It also takes salts from the lymph. It draws upon the liver for sugar, and probably for proteins also. Ina starving animal the blood still contains sugar long after fresh supplies have ceased to reach it from the intestines. The lungs remove its carbonic acid. The kidneys free it from everything which cannot be otherwise removed. It is essential to the well-being of the organism as a whole that a uniform standard of composition should be maintained by the blood. Composition.—The structural composition of the blood, and the relation of its several constituents to each other, is best studied under the microscope. A thin transparent membrane in which blood is circulating through small vessels—the web between the toes of a frog’s foot, the mesentery, the membrane of a bat’s ear—affords an opportunity of observing blood in circulation. In any of the smaller vessels, whether artery or vein, a column of red corpuscles is seen moving in the axis of the stream. This column is surrounded by a layer of clear plasma. Amongst the’red corpuscles a few leucocytes may be detected floating placidly down the current. Others are seen in the peripheral layer of plasma, tending to creep along the wall of the vessel rather than submit to be moved forward, as passive objects, by the current. If an irritant be applied to the membrane, the vessels dilate ; yet, notwithstanding their wider calibre, the current becomes slower. The red corpuscles mass together. Apparently their constitution is slightly altered by this commencing inflammation, in such a manner that they cease to be clean, independent discs which slide past each other like small boats on a river ; they exhibit a tendency to stick one to another. In the capillary vessels leucocytes may now be observed, not merely creeping along the inner surface of the endothelium, but squeezing themselves between its scales ; making their way out of the vessel into the tissue- spaces through which the vessel passes. Such an observation | ~ , e 7 : aes a er be ah aim eee yee : (te, A eT ES ee Sh id On , over | THE BODY AT WORK gives the clue to the functions of the several sonstiuenie of the blood. The red corpuscles carry oxygen in chemical com- __ bination with their colouring matter. From them it passes — into solution in the plasma; from the plasma through the walls of the capillary vessels into lymph ; the tissues take it from the lymph as they require it. As fast as it is removed from lymph it is renewed from plasma. Carbonic acid excreted by tissue-cells is dissolved in lymph. From lymph it is trans- Fia. 4.—RED BLOOD-CORPUSCLES PRESENTING, SOME THE SURFACES, OTHERS THE EDGES, OF THEIR DISOS, TOGETHER WITH SINGLE REPRESENTATIVES OF FOUR TYPES OF LEUCO- OYTE. A, the most common type, highly ameeboid and phagocytic. Its protoplasm is finely granular, its nucleus multipartite. B, a leucocyte closely similar to the last, but larger, and con- taining an undivided nucleus. It is shown with a cluster of particles of soot in its body- substance. C, a young leucocyte, or ‘‘ lymphocyte.” D, a coarsely granular leucocyte. Its granules stain brightly with acid dyes—e.g., eosin or acid fuchsin. ferred to plasma. The reception of carbonic acid by these fluids is not quite so simple as the transference of oxygen from blood to lymph. It is aided by the presence of alkaline carbo- nates which are always ready to form “acid” salts: not acid to litmus-paper—the blood is always alkaline—but containing more than one unit of acid to one of base. Sodic carbonate has the formula Na,CO,;. With an additional molecule of carbonic acid it becomes Na,CO,CO,(HO)—bicarbonate. When in solution it can hold still more carbonic acid. If carbonic acid 2 . ie re MP “tit” “aie ‘ee! ae * i? ht ESS pile iN eet ii ata ne ea a " 4 Pod ernst ' Lees Pr ay J ' r THE FLUIDS OF THE BODY 61 r ‘were merely dissolved in lymph and plasma, it would be im- _ possible for the blood to carry it away with sufficient rapidity ; just as it would be impossible for blood to bring sufficient oxygen were it not for the colouring matter (hemoglobin) which forms a temporary, easily divorced union with it. But from a physical point of view it comes to the same thing. As the tension of oxygen in plasma falls, it dissolves more from the hemoglobin. When the tension of oxygen in lymph is less than its tension in plasma, the former borrows from the latter. If the tension of carbonic acid in lymph is higher than in blood, it passes to the blood. The rapidly circulating blood at frequent intervals traverses the lungs. The whole blood of the body is exposed to air in the lungs once every minute. Oxygen tension being higher in pulmonary air than in venous blood, this gas is taken up. Carbonic acid tension being higher in venous blood than in pulmonary air, this gas escapes. The plasma in the capillary vessels which traverse the tissues exchanges gases with the lymph with very great rapidity. The specific gravity of blood varies from 1-056 to 1-059. The corpuscles are heavier than the plasma. Its reaction to test-paper is alkaline, owing to the presence of bicarbonate of soda and disodic phosphate. The alkalinity is greatest when the body is at rest ; it is diminished by severe muscular exer- cise. Blood contains about 5,000,000 red corpuscles, and 7,000 or 8,000 leucocytes, to a cubic millimetre. Red blood- corpuscles are biconcave discs destitute of nucleus, and, so far as can be seen, devoid of any investing membrane. Seen in profile they appear biscuit-shaped, because the centre is hollowed out. Their largest diameter is 7-5 micromillimetres (sso inch)—a measurement of great importance to anyone who works with a microscope, because it serves as a standard by which to estimate the size of other objects. They are soft, but fairly tough and highly elastic. In circulating blood a corpuscle may occasionally be seen to catch on the point where two capillary vessels unite. It bends almost double under the pressure of the column of corpuscles behind it, and then springs forward. A red corpuscle is a vehicle for hemoglobin. If blood is diluted with water, or if it is alternately frozen and thawed, the hemoglobin separates from the corpuscles, which can then "I a i — = See et ha 62 THE BODY AT WORK — be seen as colourless discs. Hemoglobin constitutes 40 per cent. of the weight of a moist corpuscle, or 95 per cent. of its weight after it has been dried. This is an enormous charge for a corpuscle to carry, and the question of how it carries it has been much discussed. It is not in a crystalline state. A corpuscle examined by polarized light is not doubly refrac- tive. Microscopists know that if there were any crystals in the corpuscle it would appear bright on a dark ground when the Nicholl prisms are crossed. It cannot be in solution, since the water which the corpuscle contains would not suffice to dissolve it. It must be combined with some constituent of the corpuscle. But whether it is uniformly distributed throughout the disc, or in a semifluid form enclosed in spaces in a sponge-work; or whether the corpuscle is a hollow vesicle enclosing fluid hemoglobin—a view which was long ago maintained, and has recently been revived—are questions which still await further evidence. | Red blood-corpuscles, properly so called, are found only in vertebrate animals, although invertebrate animals, from worms upwards, possess genuine blood, and in some of them it contains © hemoglobin, or a similar pigment in the form of globules. These might be likened to the non-nucleated corpuscles of mammals, but it must be remembered that the non-nucleated cells of mammals have been evolved from the nucleated blood- corpuscles of birds, reptiles, amphibians, and fishes. Below — fishes red blood-cells are not found. Hemoglobin is usually dissolved in the blood of invertebrate animals. It is impossible to trace any relationship between the coloured globules of invertebrates and the blood-cells of fishes. The coloured globules must be regarded as deposits or accretions of hemo- globin held together by a proteid substance. — The nucleated red corpuscles of submammalian vertebrates multiply by cell division while circulating in the blood-stream. A good subject in which to look for dividing corpuscles is the blood of a newt in spring-time, when rapidly increasing activity calls for an additional supply. There is nothing to distinguish the method of division of a nucleated blood-corpuscle from that of any other cell. The life-story of the red blood-corpuscles of mammals is one of the most fascinating that the histologist has to tell. He earlier Gincters: It is unlikely that a blood-corpuscle lives for long. A month or six weeks is probably the term of its exist- ence. The rapidity with which the stock is replenished after _ bleeding shows that there must be ample provision in the body _ for making blood-corpuscles. The rate at which they dis- appear after they have been added in excess shows that there is an equally effective mechanism for destroying them. If half as many again as the animal already possesses be injected into its veins, the number is reduced to its normal limit in about ten days. It is clear that they can be made and can be de- stroyed with great facility, and it seems a legitimate inference that production and destruction are constantly taking place. Regarding the way in which they are destroyed there is no uncertainty. We shall refer to this subject when describing the functions of the spleen. But how are they made? We can sketch their history in outline, but the evidence is conflicting with regard to all matters of detail. In early stages of embryonic life all red blood-corpuscles are nucleated, as they are permanently in birds and the other classes of vertebrates below mammals. In embryonic mammals they multiply by division whilst circulating in the blood, just as in the newt. But it is generally believed that this is not the most important source of new ones. During the earliest stages of growth they are being formed in enormous numbers. Such instances of division as can be seen in circulating blood appear to be all too infrequent to account for their rapid multiplica- tion, and there can be no doubt but that a more complicated method of production is more important. Their formation is described as taking place “endogenously.” Certain cells termed ‘“‘ vaso-formative,” or “‘ vaso-sanguiformative,”’ reach a considerable size, and become stellate in form, or branched. Their nuclei divide without the cell dividing. Each nucleus accumulates a little hemoglobin round it. A space filled with fluid appears inside the cell. The nuclei project into this space. Then they drop off with their envelopes of hamo- globin. The outer shell of the big vaso-formative cell becomes the wall of a capillary bloodvessel. By its branches it links up with other vaso-formative cells, making a network of vessels. ee THE BODY AT WORK The fluid inside it is the plasma of the blood. The nuclei and — their envelopes are blood-corpuscles. This, if it be a true story, is a comprehensive way of making bloodvessels and blood at the same time. Doubts have been thrown upon its accuracy, but many leading histologists strenuously maintain that this description is correct. At a certain period all nucleated red corpuscles disappear from mammalian blood. Non-nucleated corpuscles take their place. How are the latter formed? For a short stage of embryonic life nucleated cells containing blood-pigment are seen, or are supposed to be seen, in the liver—there is, unfortu- nately, great difficulty in distinguishing them with certainty from young liver-cells; later they are seen in the spleen ; throughout the whole of life they are to be seen in the marrow of bone. The nucleated cells give origin to the non-nucleated corpuscles. It is hardly legitimate to call these cells persistent embryonic corpuscles. Yet the chain which connects the cells which in the embryo are capable of dividing into pairs of nucleated red blood-corpuscles, and the cells which, assuming the réle of parent cells, do not accumulate hemoglobin for their own purposes, but for the benefit of the red corpuscles which split off from them, is probably unbroken. In this sense they are persistent embryonic corpuscles which have deserted the blood-stream, and have taken shelter in certain tissues which are particularly favourable for cell division. The situations in which they hide themselves are singularly suggestive. In the liver there is an abundant supply of nutriment, more abun- dant than in any other part of the body of the embryo. Later, in the spleen, red blood-corpuscles are being destroyed. Materials available for making new ones must therefore be set free. The inside of a hollow bone is a peculiarly sheltered situation. The fat cells of marrow accumulate there after a time ; but within some bones the marrow develops very little fat ; hence it shows the red colour, which is due to its abundant bloodvessels. This “‘red marrow” is the most important seat of the manufacture of red blood-corpuscles in adult life. Unfortunately, when we try to answer the question, How are they formed ? we are obliged to speak with caution. Some histologists assert that the nucleated cells divide, and that one of the two daughter cells accumulates hemoglobin, and hat is to say, extrudes—its nucleus. Others maintain at the nucleated cells become irregular in form ; that hemo- a" obin accumulates in the projecting portion of the cell ; that _ this projecting portion breaks off as a non-nucleated corpuscle, _ It would be indiscreet at the present time to pronounce in _ favour of either of these reports, although the decision is of _ theoretical importance. If the former account be true, red _ blood-corpuscles are nucleated blood-cells which have lost their nuclei. If the latter account be in accordance with fact, it is hardly justifiable to regard them as cells. They are parts of cells which finish their existence independently of the cell body and nucleus to which they belong. As circumstantial evidence, favouring the theory that cell division is normal and the nucleus subsequently lost, may be pleaded the exist- ence in marrow, and also in the embryonic liver and spleen, of certain very peculiar cells. These cells have long been known as giant cells, and all attempts at accounting for them have broken down. They are relatively of immense size: their diameter may be twenty times as great as that of a red blood -corpuscle. Each contains a huge irregular, bulging nucleus. Hence the cells are termed “‘ megacaryocytes ”’ (big- nucleus cells). They must not be confounded with the poly- caryocytes (cells with several nuclei), which eat up degrading bone, although it must be confessed that megacaryocytes and polycaryocytes appear to be genetically connected. It is sup- posed that megacaryocytes consume the nuclei which red corpuscles extrude during the process of their conversion from nucleated cells. Traces of nuclei, or things which often look like nuclei, are found in their body-substance. Their own over- grown misformed nuclei appear to be the result of an excess of nuclear food. It is certainly remarkable that megacaryo- cytes are not found below mammals. They do not occur in any animal in which red blood-corpuscles retain their nuclei. Polycaryocytes are found in numbers in the bones of growing birds. They are evidently scooping out bone from situations in which it has to be displaced in order that the shape of the bone as a whole may be changed. But there are no mega- caryocytes in birds. On the other hand, megacaryocytes are present in the liver, and later in the spleen, of mammals at the periods when blood-formation is occurring most actively in 5 5 a OEY BODY igi i ae these organs. From the liver they disappear aie In nn mammals they disappear from the spleen about the time « g birth ; but in some—the hedgehog, for example—they are found i in the spleen throughout the whole of life. | ‘Hemoglobin is a substance which has the property of uniting — with oxygen to form oxyhemoglobin—a compound from which ~ the oxygen is, again, very readily withdrawn. It is extremely — soluble, but may be made to crystallize by adding alcohol to blood, after setting the hemoglobin free from the corpuscles by freezing and thawing. From the blood of Man and most other animals it crystallizes in the form of rhombic prisms, whether in the oxidized (oxyhemoglobin) or non-oxidized condition. The addition of oxygen does not affect its crystal- line form ; although crystalline, it is absolutely non-diffusible. This is due to the great size of its molecule, which is probably larger than that of any other substance which is capable of crystallizing. The percentage composition of hemoglobin conforms closely with that of albumin and other proteins, with this most im- portant difference : it contains a definite proportion of iron— 0-336 per cent. That the percentage of carbon, hydrogen, nitrogen, sulphur, and oxygen should agree with that com- monly found in proteins is inevitable, since it may be split into a part which contains all the iron, hematin, and a proteid part resembling albumin; and the latter constitutes 96 per cent. of its weight. There is no doubt but that its value as a vehicle of oxygen depends upon the presence of iron. In the matter of taking up and dropping oxygen, hematin behaves somewhat in the same manner as hemoglobin; whereas if iron be removed from hematin the “ iron-free hematin ”’ loses its respiratory value. It is almost certain that a molecule of hemoglobin contains a single atom of iron. On this supposition its molecular formula may be calculated. It is not quite the same for all animals, although the variations are slight. For the blood of the horse it is as follows : Cr aH y330N a1 FeO os. This means a molecular weight of 16708. We give the figures, because the properties of hemoglobin will be better understood Pee Port ee ate mai meee 67 is prodigious molecular gaia; is borne in nied In a sense, 1e reason for the great size of its molecule is not far to seek. & | The atomic weight of iron (Fe = 56) is much greater than that _ of either of the other elements contained in hemoglobin. The 3 molecule needs to be very great to float an atom of iron. As it is, the corpuscles are heavier than the plasma which sur- _ rounds them, in the proportion of about 13 to 12. Although hemoglobin is a crystallizable substance, its immense molecule is absolutely non-diffusible. It cannot pass through a mem- brane. This is of no consequence as regards the relation of hemoglobin to the walls of the capillary bloodvessels, since it is contained in corpuscles; but it is of great importance as regards its relation to the discs which carry it. A very small quantity of enveloping substance suffices to prevent it from diffusing into the plasma of the blood. The great molecules are held together and isolated from the fluid in which they float by a minimal amount of insoluble globin. The iron needed for the making of hemoglobin is obtained both from meat and vegetables. The constituents of an ordinary diet provide from 2 to 3 centigrammes of iron a day. The whole of the blood contains about 4:5 grammes. When corpuscles are being destroyed in the spleen, the iron which their pigment contains is largely reabsorbed and rendered available for further use. The iron in a mixed diet is more than sufficient to counterbalance any loss. Milk contains extremely little iron. Before birth the liver and spleen accu- mulate a store of iron which lasts until the end of the nursing period, unless this be unduly prolonged. If it be prolonged, the child is apt to become anemic. Iron has been administered in the treatment of anzmia ever since its presence in the red clot of blood was recognized a hundred and fifty years ago. Physi- cians are agreed that in the anemia of young people it is of value ; but observations made with a view to obtaining definite data as to the increase in number of blood-corpuscles which results from the administration of iron, without any other alteration in the diet or the habits of the patient, have not given accordant results. Some observers have obtained an increase with organic compounds of iron, others with inorganic com- pounds ; some are in favour of small doses, others of very large ones. As in the treatment by drugs of other abnormal condi- 5—2 i 68 THE BODY AT WORK _ tions, it is difficult to isolate the effect of the drug from the _ effects of improvements in the general regimen. Yet physi- cians agree that iron accentuates the beneficial ee of fresh | air and improved diet. . ‘When the surface of the body is struck, the effect of the blow is marked at first by redness. There is nothing to show that small bloodvessels have been ruptured and blood effused beneath the skin. Next day the injured area is reddish-purple. The bruise turns blue, green, yellow, and eventually disappears. In the process of absorption, oxyhemoglobin undergoes de- composition. First its proteid constituent is removed, leaving - a coloured pigment containing iron, termed “ hematin ’’; soon reduced by loss of oxygen to hemochromogen. When Sir George Stokes first described the spectrum of blood (cf. p. 185), he showed that as hemoglobin may exist in an oxidized and in a non-oxidized condition, distinguished by their spectra, so also may the coloured residue which is left after the proteid constituent has been removed from hemoglobin. This coloured residue he termed, when oxidized, ‘*‘ hematin’ ; when not oxidized, ‘“‘ reduced hematin.’ Stokes’s reduced hematin is now termed “ hemochromogen.” Hemochromogen stands for the coloured nucleus of hemoglobin. Although it is not present in hemoglobin as hemochromogen—hence we must not speak of hemoglobin as made of a protein, x, plus hemo- chromogen, y—it is to its coloured residue that hemoglobin owes its value as a carrier of oxygen. Later, iron is removed from hemochromogen, leaving hematoidin, a substance often found at the seat of old hemorrhages, where it may remain unchanged for a very long time. Hematoidin is apparently identical with the yellow pigment of bile, bilirubin. The green colour which shows itself in the bruise seems to indicate that the more oxidized bile-pigment, biliverdin, is formed in the first instance. Red corpuscles, when destroyed in the spleen, pass through transformations similar to those which blood undergoes when effused beneath the skin. Their protein is used by the phagocytes which eat them. Their iron is reserved for the use of the blood-forming cells of the red marrow of bone. The pigment which remains as the residue of hemoglobin is carried by the splenic vein to the liver, which secretes it as bile-pigment. So much of the bile-pigment as is Se = Gia S Ye et wee re, Ie pa F LIE erat? Frye” ocr a # $ Ao ' s iy oA “FLUIDS oF THE BODY 69 E Zabsoxbea 1 by the wall of the alimentary canal is eventually — a ‘cxorted as the pigment of urine. Such is the history of the changes which blood-pigment _ undergoes within the living body. To a certain extent its _ chemistry can be followed in the laboratory ; but it must be remembered, when we are treating of the chemistry of a sub- stance as complex as hemoglobin, that the products which can be obtained from it in the laboratory are not necessarily those into which it is transformed in the body. In the laboratory oxyhzemoglobin is easily changed into methemoglobin, a sub- stance of the same percentage composition, but with its oxygen more firmly fixed. Methemoglobin can be decomposed into a proteid substance and hematin. Hematin, when acted on by reducing agents, becomes hemochromogen. Hemochromo- gen, when subjected to such a reducing agent as a mixture of tin and hydrochloric acid, gives rise to coloured bodies closely resembling bile-pigments—not as they are secreted by the bile, but as they appear in the urine. It is impossible to prove that the changing colours of a bruise indicate a sequence of chemical transformations from hzemoglobin to bile-pigment, but it is not improbable that such a description is correct. The test commonly used to ascertain the presence of bile-pigment, e., bilirubin, is the play of colours which it exhibits when oxidized by fuming nitric acid. From yellow it turns to green, to blue, and then to purple, more or less reversing the colours of the bruise. It is fairly certain that effused blood undergoes changes along lines which, if not identical with those through which blood passes on its road to bile-pigment, are at any rate very similar. Coagulation of Lymph and Blood.—Two or three minutes after blood has been shed it begins to clot. In ten minutes the vessel into which it has been received may be inverted without spilling the blood. After a time the jelly, holding all the corpuscles, shrinks from the sides of the jar. It squeezes out a transparent, straw-coloured fluid—serum. The clot con- tinues to contract until, in a few hours, about one-half of the weight of the blood is clot, the other half serum. Lymph coagulates like blood, but most specimens clot more slowly, and the product is less firm. When the process is watched through the microscope—a 4 : | > > gw

+,” Se Oe oe te es 5 ve fs c1-< A, in > oh tie eae ee “ Panes, +r Wes THE BODY AT WORK few drops of the almost colourless, transparent blood of a — lobster afford an excellent opportunity of studying the forma- tion of the clot—innumerable filaments of the most delicate description are seen to shoot out from many centres. They multiply until they constitute a felt-work. In the case of blood obtained from a vertebrate animal, this felt-work holds the corpuscles in its meshes. Its filaments exhibit a remarkable tendency to ccntract. They shorten as much as the enclosed corpuscles allow. The filaments may be prevented from entangling the cor- puscles by whipping the blood, from the instant that it is shed, with a bundle of twigs or wires. The fibrin collects on the wires, while the corpuscles remain in the serum. If this fibrin is washed in running water until all adherent serum and cor- puscles are removed, it appears as a soft white stringy sub- stance which, when dried, resembles isinglass. Clotting is a protection against hemorrhage. As it oozes from a scratch or tiny wound, blood clots, forming a natural plaster which prevents continued bleeding. It has little if any influence in resisting a strongly flowing stream of blood. But a clean cut through a large vessel is an accident which rarely happens as the result of natural causes. It is not the kind of injury to which animals are liable. When an artery is severed by a blunt instrument, the muscle-fibres of its wall contract. They occlude the vessel. The blood clots at the place where the vessel is injured, and plugs it. This happens also when a surgeon ties an artery. He is careful to pull the ligature suffi- ciently tight to crush its wall. His sensitive fingers feel it give. He stops before the thread has cut it through. As will be ex- plained later, the clotting of blood is promoted by contact with injured tissue. If in tying an artery its wall be not crushed, the blood in it may remain liquid. When it is skilfully tied, the blood clots, forming a firm plug which is practically a part of the artery, by the time that the silk thread used in tying it is thrown out, owing to the death of the ring of tissue which it compressed. After a tooth has been extracted, the cavity is closed and further bleeding stopped by clotted blood. When large vessels have been severed, the copious heemor- rhage which follows induces fainting. For a short time the heart stops, or beats very feebly. The blood-pressure falls. Ns a i a “of coming into cs It is a useful reflex action, always sup- posing that the person who is liable to it faints at the sight of _ his own blood. Amongst other reasons for the greater fortitude _ of women—they are far less subject to this emotional reflex than men—might be alleged the circumstances of life of primi- tive people. It was the part of their women-folk to dress wounds, not to receive them. The phenomenon of coagulation has attracted attention from the earliest times. It was a phenomenon that needed explanation, and culinary experience suggested analogies close at hand. Hippocrates attributed the clotting of blood to its coming to rest and growing cold. The blood which gushed from a warrior’s wound formed a still pool by his side. It set into a jelly as it cooled. Until the second quarter of the nine- teenth century this theory was deemed sufficient. It then occurred to two men of inquiring mind to institute control experiments. John Davy placed a dish of blood upon the hob. William Hunter kept one shaking. In both experiments the blood clotted more quickly than it did in vessels of the same size, containing the same amount of the same blood, left upon the table. Even before this date an observation had been made regard- ing the circumstances in which clotting occurs, which has thrown much light upon the causes of the phenomenon. In 1772 Hewson gently tied a vein in two places. At the end of a couple of hours he opened the vein. The blood was still liquid, but clotted in a normal manner after it was shed. Scudamore showed that blood clots more slowly in a closed than in an open flask. A new theory, as little trustworthy as Hippo- crates’, was based upon these observations. Blood clotted because it was exposed to air. A record of all observations of the circumstances of coagulation, and of all the theories to which they have given rise, would make an exceptionally interesting chapter in the history of human thought. It would bring into singular prominence stages in the development of what is now known as the “scientific method.’ Not that Science has a method of her own. Philosophers of all classes would follow ait es can be brought to a test of HGR. the ‘ political, or economic theory are not susceptit confronted with control experiments. The control ext is the alphabet and the syntax of the scientific method. hypothesis is admissible into the pyramid of theory until it has _ passed this test. A natural phenomenon is observed. Every measurement which is applicable is taken and recorded—time, _ weight, temperature, colour. Scientific observation implies the tabulation of all particulars which are capable of statistical expression. Reflecting upon the relation of the phenomenonto other phenomena of a like nature, the philosopher—it is the philosophy of physiologists which interests us—formulates an hypothesis as to its cause. At this point the real difficulty of —_ applying the scientific method begins. It is easy to formulate hypotheses. It is very difficult to devise control experiments. An experiment must be arranged which will provide that, while all other conditions in which the phenomenon has been observed to occur are reproduced, the condition which was ez hypothes: its cause shall be omitted. This digression into the philosophy of science may seem to be somewhat remote from our line of march,. but it may perhaps hasten our progress in the comprehension of the story of physiology. There is no other science in which the control experiment plays an equally important part. Unless this is realized, the whole trend of experimental work will be misunderstood. Scudamore ex- plained coagulation as due to contact with air. Based on the observations we have cited, no hypothesis could have seemed more reasonable. With a view to checking this hypothesis, blood was received into a tube of mercury. It coagulated in the Torricellian vacuum. Scudamore’s hypothesis, like many earlier and later, when confronted with a control experiment, was turned away, ashamed. Clotting is a property of plasma. Red corpuscles play no part in the process. Coagulation does not occur in a living healthy vessel. It occurs when the vessel, and especially when its inner coat, is injured. It is hastened by contact with wounded tissues, especially with wounded skin. Contact with a foreign body also starts coagulation. If a silk thread is ee Z Be on the ves! by the needle which was used to draw it _ through. ee a ed ee a get ron 2 te Plasma contains a substance which sets into fibrin. It has been termed “fibrinogen.” It is present in lymph, and in almost all forms of exuded lymph. If sodium chloride (common salt) is added to plasma until it is half saturated—until it has dissolved half as much as the maximum quantity which it can dissolve—fibrinogen is thrown down as a flocculent pre- cipitate. It can be redissolved and reprecipitated until it is pure. When fibrinogen was separated from plasma a step was taken towards the explanation of coagulation. Under certain conditions fibrinogen sets into fibrin. The question which then presented itself for solution was as follows : What is the substance which, by acting upon or combining with fibrinogen, converts it into fibrin? The clue to the solution of this question was obtained from the consideration of certain observations made by Andrew Buchanan in 1830, but long neglected, because their significance was not understood. Buchanan had observed that some specimens of lymph exuded into a lymph-space—the peritoneal cavity, for example—will clot ; others will not. He noticed that they clot when, owing to puncture of a small bloodvessel during the process of drawing them off, they are tinged with blood. Determined to ascertain which of the constituents of blood is effective in rendering non- coagulable effusions capable of clotting, he added to them in turn red blood-corpuscles, serum, and the washings of blood- clot. Either of the two latter was found to contain the clot- provoking substance. Thirty years later a German physiolo- gist prepared fibrinogen from effused lymph by precipitating it with salt. He also treated serum in a similar way, precipitating a protein which he termed fibrinoplastin. When these two sub- stances were dissolved and the solutions mixed, he obtained a clot, which he regarded as a compound of fibrinogen and fibrino- plastin. Subsequently he found that the mixture did not always clot, but he discovered that if he coagulated blood with alcohol, and washed this residue, the washings added to the mixed solution just referred to invariably produced a clot. Thinking that the substance which he obtained from his 14 “‘ fibrin-ferment.” He neglected the control experiment. He failed to ascertain whether or not all three substances were needed. Had he tried adding fibrin-ferment to fibrinogen, he would havediscovered that the further addition of fibrinoplastin was unnecessary. He did not ascertain, as he might have done, that the weight of fibrin formed is somewhat less, not greater, than the weight of fibrinogen used. (Fibrinogen gives off a certain quantity of globulin when it changes into fibrin.) He was also wrong in supposing that the water which he added to alcohol-coagulated blood dissolved no protein. His “ fibrin- ferment ”’ is always associated with a protein. Since it may also be obtained from lymphatic glands, thymus gland, and other tissues which contain lymphocytes, it has been inferred that it is itself a protein, of the class known as nucleo-proteins. The fact that it is destroyed at so low a temperature as 55° C. has been supposed to confirm the theory that it is a protein. But with regard to the chemical nature of fibrin-ferment, as of all other ferments, we are at present in the dark. Under ordinary circumstances, when blood clots, the fibrin-ferment, or plasmase, or thrombin—it has received various names—is set free by leucocytes. Fluids which contain fibrinogen clot on the addition of a “‘ ferment ” which is either secreted by leuco- cytes or set free from leucocytes when they break up—as they are very apt to do, as soon as the conditions upon which their health depends are interfered with. Freshly shed blood contains minute particles, termed “ plate- lets,’ in diameter measuring about a quarter that of a red blood- corpuscle. When the inner coat of a vessel is injured, platelets accumulate at the injured spot. They form a little white heap, from which coagulation starts. Evidently they supply the ferment, or a precursor of the ferment. As yet their origin has not been traced. They are too large to be the unchanged granules of granular leucocytes, but that they are in some way derived from leucocytes seems probable. The further study of coagulation has shown that the con- ditions under which it occurs are more complicated than the simple explanation just given would seem to imply. This explanation holds good, so far as it goes, but facts connected with the details of the process have recently been brought to - aleohol-coagulated blood could not be proteid, he termed it F ‘THE FLUIDS OF THE BODY 15 i light which warn the physiologist that as yet his theory of coagulation is incomplete. The presence of salts of lime has an important relation to coagulation. If blood is received into a vessel in which has been placed some powdered oxalate of potash, or soap, or any other chemical which fixes lime, the blood does not coagulate. All other conditions are as usual, but lime is withdrawn from the plasma. The non-coagulation of oxalated plasma was interpreted as indicating that lime, under the influence of fibrin-ferment, combines with fibrinogen to form fibrin; that fibrinogen altered by fibrin-ferment combines with lime. This hypothesis was based upon the analogy of the curdling of milk. Milk cannot curdle if lime be absent. If rennin (milk- ferment), prepared from milk from which lime has been re- moved, be added to a solution of caseinogen (the coagulable protein of milk), also prepared from lime-free milk, no curd is produced. The addition of a few drops of a solution of chloride of lime results in the immediate curdling of the mixture. Evidently rennin so alters caseinogen as to bring it into a condition to combine with lime. But the analogy does not hold good for blood. In the case of plasma, lime acts, not upon fibrinogen, but upon the fibrin-ferment—or rather upon a precursor of fibrin-ferment—in such a way as to render it effective. Leucocytes produce a prothrombin, which in contact with lime-salts is converted into thrombin, which coagulates fibrinogen. Fibrinogen is the substance which fibrin-ferment combined with salts of lime changes into fibrin. Yet even now the story is not complete, if the theory of coagulation is to be brought up to date. A perfectly clean cannula is passed into an artery of a bird. If it be thrust well beyond the place where the vessel has been cut, if the vessel be tied so gently as to avoid injury to its inner coat, and if the blood which first passes through the cannula be allowed to escape, the blood subsequently collected will not clot. It contains fibrinogen, lime salts, and fibrin-ferment, ordinarily so called; but the ferment is ineffective. The addition to the blood of a frag- ment of injured tissue, or of a watery extract of almost any tissue, immediately sets up coagulation. This observation brings fibrin-ferment into line with other ferments. Digestive dacneed by a kinase before ere acquire Sechaniative ys _ So, too, must thrombogen be changed into thrombin, under — the influence of thrombokinase, before it can act upon fibrinogen. Almost all tissues yield the kinase which actuates fibrin-ferment. The utility of this provision is manifest. A bird’s blood contains everything necessary to form a clot with the exception of thrombokinase. The injury which brings the blood into contact with a broken surface supplies this ferment of the ferment. Fibrin-ferment, rendered active, at once — changes fibrinogen into fibrin. The same interaction is neces- sary before the blood of a mammal is susceptible of clotting. But a mammal’s blood is even readier to clot than is the blood of a bird ; for not only will a broken surface provide it with thrombokinase, but the leucocytes contained within the blood, when injured, also yield it. And the leucocytes are exceed- ingly sensitive of any change of circumstance ; on the slightest indication that conditions are not normal they set free, perhaps owing to their own disintegration, the kinase which turns thrombogen into thrombin. There is a constitutional condition, fortunately rare, in which blood does not coagulate. A person subject to this abnormality is said to suffer from hemophilia. It is alleged that this condition is due to deficiency of lime in the blood ; and the deficiency of lime is said to be due to excess of phos- phates. The subject suffers from phosphaturia. His kidneys get rid of the superabundance of phosphates by excreting them in combination with lime. If this explanation be correct, there is a chronic insufficiency of lime in the blood, because it is being constantly withdrawn in the process of removing phosphates. The difficulty in the way of establishing a complete theory of the coagulation of blood increases when the phenomena of incoagulability are considered. Blood may be rendered in- capable of clotting in a variety of ways. Leeches and other animals which suck blood have the capacity of rendering it incoagulable. If the heads are removed from a score of leeches, thrown into absolute alcohol, dried, ground in a pepper mill, extracted with normal saline solution, a dark turbid liquor is obtained. This liquor, after filtration and ss THE FLUIDS OF THE BODY 77 sterilization at a temperature of 120° C., injected into the veins of an animal, renders its blood incoagulable. The preparation sold by druggists under the name “ pep- tone,” when injected into the veins of a dog, renders its blood incoagulable. Commercial “‘ peptone”’ is a mixture of many substances. Its anticoagulation-effect is not due to the peptone which it contains. It has been supposed to be due to imperfectly digested albumin and gelatin (proteoses), but products of bacteric fermentation (toxins and ptomaines) are more probably the active bodies. Not only is the peptonized blood of a dog incoagulable, but if this blood be injected into the veins of a rabbit (an animal upon which the direct injec- tion of peptone has no effect), it diminishes the coagula- bility of the rabbit’s blood. If peptonized blood be mixed in a beaker with non-peptonized blood, it prevents the coagula- tion of the latter. There is little doubt but that the poison, whatever it may be, acts upon the leucocytes ; and there are some reasons for thinking that the poison is not contained in the “ peptone,” but is secreted by the liver of the animal into which the “ peptone ”’ has been injected. A still more remarkable property in relation to coagula- tion must be assigned to leucocytes. The blood of a dog which has been rendered incoagulable by injection of peptone recovers its coagulability after a time. If a further injection of “ peptone’ be made, the animal is found to be immune. Injection of “ peptone ” no longer renders its blood incoagu- lable. In a similar manner the blood develops a power of resisting the action of agents which induce its coagulation whilst circulating in the vascular system. Nucleo-proteins contained in extracts of lymphatic glands and other organs when injected into the veins of living animals cause their blood to clot, provided they are injected in sufficient quantity. If they are injected in quantity less than sufficient to induce coagulation, they render the animal immune to their influence. A larger quantity given to an animal thus prepared fails to take effect. This brings the phenomena of coagulation and resistance to coagulation to the verge of chemistry. They extend into the domain in which pathology reigns. Tempting though it be to record other facts with regard to these pheno- mena which recent investigation has brought to light, it is THE BODY. aT WORK ne probably judicious to leave the problem at the frontier. re ross the frontier lies a fascinating land, rich with unimaginable — a 4 possibilities for the human race. Settlement is rapidly pro- ceeding in this country, which is charted, like other border- lands, with barbarous names: “antibodies,” “‘haptors,” — “amboceptors,” ‘“‘ toxins,” “ antitoxins,” and the like— finger-posts to hypotheses which show every sign of hasty and provisional construction. But certain facts stand out, in whatever way theory may, in the future, link them up. The virus of hydrophobia, modified by passing through a rabbit, develops in human beings, even when injected after they have been infected, the power of resisting hydrophobia. The serum of a horse which has acquired immunity to diphtheria aids the blood of a child, which has not had time to become immune, in destroying the germs of this disease. It is a contest between the blood and offensive bodies of all kinds which find en- trance to it, whether living germs or poisons in solution ; with victory always, in the long-run, on the side of the blood, pro- vided its owner does not die in the meantime. And not only is the blood victorious in the struggle with any given invader, but having repulsed him, it retains for a long while a property which neutralizes all further attempts at aggression on his part. In the past, physicians have fought disease with such clumsy weapons as mercury, arsenic, and quinine. Now they anticipate disease. In mimic warfare with an attenuated virus the blood is trained to combat. Smallpox which has been passed through the body of a cow is suppressed by the blood’s native strength. The exercise develops skill to deal with the most virulent germs of the same kind. In cases in which physicians cannot anticipate disease in human beings, they train the blood of animals to meet it ; and, keeping their serum in stock, they can, when the critical moment arrives, reinforce the fighting strength of the patient with this mer- cenary aid. The Spleen.—The spleen is placed on the left side of the body, and rather towards the back. It rests between the stomach and the inner surface of the eighth, ninth, tenth, and eleventh ribs. It is quickly distinguished from other organs by its brown-purple colour, a sombre hue to which it owed its evil reputation with the humoralists. The liver’s yellow bile ee eee! oe A eee ey key a oe ge a Me, yr “Se a le 5 ee BS pg ll nt I Oe f ee Bee ee a ‘THE FLUIDS OF THE BODY 79 tin yed man’s mental outlook, preventing him from seeing Zz piece | in their natural brightness ; but the spleen made black bile, which, mounting to the brain, displayed its malign influence upon the action of that organ, as, or in, the worst of humours. The spleen is invested with a capsule of no great toughness. Inside the capsule is “‘ spleen pulp.””’ When the fresh organ is cut across, it is seen that, although most of the pulp is of the colour of dark venous blood, it is mottled with light patches. In some -animals—the cat, for example—these whitish patches are small round spots, regularly arranged at a certain distance from the capsule. The distinction into “ red pulp ” and “white pulp” marks a division into two kinds of tissue with entirely different functions. The white pulp is lymphoid tissue, lymph-follicles developed in the outer or connective-tissue coat of the branches of the splenic artery. Its function is to make lymphocytes, of which, for reasons which will shortly appear, the spleen needs an abundant supply. The constitution of the red pulp is entirely different, and peculiar to the spleen. The branches of the splenic artery divide in the usual way into smaller and still smaller twigs until the finest arterioles are reached ; but these arterioles do not give rise to capillary vessels. At the point at which in any other organ their branches would attain the calibre of capillaries, the connective-tissue cells which make their walls scatter into a reticulum. They are no longer tiles with closely fitting, sinuous, dovetailed borders, but stellate cells with long delicate processes uniting to constitute a network. The blood which the arterioles bring to the pulp is not con- ducted by closed capillary vessels across the pulp to the commencing splenic veins. It falls into the general sponge- work. The venules commence exactly in the same way as the arterioles end. Stellate connective-tissue cells become flat tiles placed edge to edge. The endothelium of an arteriole might be likened to a column of men marching shoulder to shoulder, three or four abreast ; the connective tissue of the pulp, to a crowd in an open place. The column breaks up into a crowd. On the other side the crowd falls into rank as the endothelium of veins. The capsule and the red pulp are largely composed of muscle-fibres. These relax and contract about once a minute. By their contraction ae bloc 00 squeezed out of the sponge. If the spleen be enclosed in an air-tight box (an oncometer), - ) from which a tube leads to a pressure-gauge—a drum covered a with thin membrane on which the end of a lever rests, or w a bent column of mercury on which it floats—the pressure- gauge shows the changes in volume of the spleen. The long — end of the lever, which records the variations of pressure in the gauge, may be made to scratch a line on a soot-blackened surface of travelling paper. A record of the variations in volume of the organ, which can be studied at leisure, is thus obtained. It shows that the spleen is sensitive to every change of pressure in the splenic artery. Small notches on the tracing correspond to the beats of the heart. Larger curves record the changes of blood-pressure due to respiration. A long slow rise and fall marks the rhythmic dilation and contraction of the spleen itself. One of the three large arteries into which the cceliac axis divides delivers blood to the spleen direct from the aorta. The splenic vein joins the portal vein shortly before it enters the liver. Thus the spleen is placed on a big vascular loop which directs blood, not long after it has left the heart, from the aorta, through the spleen, to the liver. The peculiar construction of the splenic pulp which brings the blood more or less to rest within its sponge-work, and the transmission to the liver of the blood which leaves the spleen, indicate that it is an organ in which blood itself receives some kind of treatment. It is not passed through it, as it is through all other parts of the body, in closed pipes. The spleen is a reservoir, or a filter- bed, into which blood is received. The red blood-corpuscles of mammals are cells without nuclei, and with little, if any, body-protoplasm. They are merely vehicles for carrying hemoglobin. We should deny to them the status of cell, if it were possible to prescribe the limit at which a structural unit ceases to be entitled to rank as a cell. They are helpless creatures, incapable of renewing their substance or of making good any of the damage to which the vicissitudes of their ceaseless circulation render them peculiarly liable. It is impossible to say with any approach ary _ THE FLUIDS OF THE BODY 81 J Ete aie aie to accuracy how long they last, but probably their average duration is comparatively short. The spleen is a labyrinth of tissue-spaces through which at frequent intervals all red corpuscles float. If they are clean, firm, resilient, they pass through without interference. If obsolete they are broken up. In the recesses of the spleen-pulp, leucocytes overtake the laggards of the blood-fleet, attach their pseudopodia to Fia. 5.—A MINUTE PORTION OF THE PULP OF THE SPLEEN, VERY HIGHLY MAGNIFIED. Stellate connective-tissue cells form spaces containing red blood-corpuscles and leucocytes. In the centre of the diagram is shown the mode of origin of a venule. It contains two phagocytes—the upper with a nucleus, two blood-corpuscles just ingested, and one partially digested in its body-substance ; the lower with two blood-corpuscles. them, draw them into their body-substance, digest them. The albuminous constituent of hzmoglobin they use, pre- sumably, for their own nutrition. The iron-containing colour- ing matter they decompose, and excrete in two parts; the iron (perhaps combined with protein) ; the colouring matter, without iron, as the pigment, or an antecedent of the pigment, which the liver will excrete in bile. Hemoglobin is un- 6 =e THE BODY AT WORK doubtedly the source of bilirubin, and general considerations lead to the conclusion that it is split into protein, iron, and iron-free pigment in the spleen ; but the details of this process have never been checked by chemical analysis. Neither bile- pigment nor an iron compound can be detected in the blood of the splenic vein. The only evidence of the setting free of iron in the spleen is to be found in the fact that the spleen yields on analysis an exceptionally large quantity of this metal (the liver also yields iron), and that the quantity is greatest - when red corpuscles are being rapidly destroyed. As a rule, it is very difficult to detect leucocytes in the act of eating red corpuscles; but under various circumstances their activity in this respect may be stimulated to such a degree as to show them, in a microscopic preparation, busily engaged in this operation. The writer had the good fortune to prepare a spleen which proved to be peculiarly suitable for this observation (Fig. 5). His method was an example of the way in which a physiological experiment ought not to be conducted. Having placed a cannula in the aorta of a rabbit, just killed with chloroform, he was proceeding to wash the blood out of its bloodvessels with a stream of warm normal saline solution, when the bottle from which the salt-solution was flowing over- turned. Fearing lest an air-bubble should enter the cannula, he hastily poured warm water into the pressure bottle, and threw in some salt, in the hope that it would make a solution of about 0-9 per cent. The salt-solution was allowed to run through the bloodvessel for rather more than an hour. When sections of the spleen were cut, after suitable hardening, every section was found to be packed with leucocytes gorged with red corpuscles. Some of the corpuscles had just been ingested ; from others the hemoglobin had already been removed. It may be that, for some unknown reason, the destruction of red corpuscles was occurring in this particular rabbit with unusual rapidity at the time when it was killed ; but it seems more probable that the animal’s leucocytes were provoked to excessive activity by changes in the red cor- puscles brought about by salt-solution which was either more or less than “‘ tonic.”” Asa score of attempts to reproduce the experiment, with solutions of different strengths, have failed, it is impossible to be sure that this is a valid explanation. ap OD Rp eS ee ee UD Be td de ear re i A ieee a. “ee _ ‘THE FLUIDS OF THE BODY $3. There must be something in the condition of worn-out red corpuscles which either makes them peculiarly attractive to predatory leucocytes or renders them an exceptionally easy prey. It does not require much imagination to picture the drama which is enacted in the spleen. Slow-moving leuco- cytes are feeling for their food. The majority of red cor- puscles pass by them ; a few are held back. The leucocytes, like children in a cake-shop, cannot consume all the buns. A selection must be made, and preference is given to the sticky, sugary ones. Red corpuscles when out of order show a tendency to stick together. When blood is stagnating in a vein, or lying on a glass slide in a layer thin enough for microscopic examination, its red discs are seen after a time to adhere together in rouleaux. The parable of a child in a cake- shop is not so fanciful as it may appear. The differentiation of function of organs is not as sharp as was formerly supposed. Evidence of their interdependence is rapidly accumulating. The activity of various organs is known to result in the formation of by-products termed ‘internal secretions,’ which influence the activity of other organs, or even of the body as a whole. The spleen enlarges after meals. This may be merely connected with the en- gorgement of the abdominal viscera which occurs during active digestion, or it may indicate, as some physiologists hold, that an internal secretion of the spleen aids the pancreas in pre- paring its ferments. The spleen enlarges greatly in ague and in some other diseases of microbial origin. This has been regarded as evidence that it takes some part in protecting the body against microbes. But whatever may be the accessory functions which it exercises, they are not of material import- ance to the organism as a whole, seeing that removal of. the spleen causes no permanent inconvenience either to men or animals. Its blood-destroying functions are taken on by accessory spleens, if there be any, and by lymphatic glands. The marrow of bone also becomes redder and more active. Under certain circumstances, red corpuscles, or fragments of red corpuscles, are to be seen within liver-cells; but it is uncertain whether blood-destruction is a standing function of the liver. 6—2 CHAPTER V INTERNAL SECRETIONS Thyroid Gland.—On either side of the windpipe, rather below the thyroid cartilage (Adam’s apple), lies a somewhat conical mass of tissue. The two masses are connected by an isthmus ; lobes and isthmus make up the thyroid gland. The whole weighs about an ounce. In health it is so soft that only the finger of an anatomist could detect it through the skin and the thin flat muscles which connect the hyoid bone and the thyroid cartilage with the breast-bone. It makes no visible prominence on the front of the neck. The thyroid gland is, however, liable to enlargement, especially amongst the people who live in certain districts. In the Valais, “ goitre,” as it is termed, is so frequent that anyone walking up the Rhone Valley is sure to meet a number of persons—for the most part women— whose swollen necks overhang their collar-bones, like half- filled sacks. Goitre is even more common in the Valle d’Aosta, on the Italian side of the Alps. In England this condition, comparatively rare, is known as “ Derbyshire ”’ or “ Hunting- donshire ”’ neck. In the majority of cases the tumour in the neck develops slowly, and does not reach its full dimensions until after middle life. Goitre in this form, although inconvenient, causes no serious discomfort. But when it appears in early life, it is associated with an extraordinary complex of malforma- tions and ill-performed functions. The condition into which a goitrous child sinks is known as cretinism. With the exception of the skull-case, its skeleton does not attain to its proper proportions ; and, since the soft parts do not equally submit to arrest of growth, the dwarf is heavy and ungainly, with large jowl and protuberant abdomen. The appearance 84, ~ INTERNAL SECRETIONS 85 of distortion is extraordinarily heightened by hypertrophy of the skin and the subcutaneous connective tissue. Ears, eye- : lids, nose, lips, fingers, are thick and heavy. The hair and nails F are coarse. The skin is folded, wrinkled, rough. The bodily ungainliness of a cretin has its counterpart in the deformity of his mind. He is an idiot whose deficiency is chiefly marked by apathy. Cretinism exhibits itself in varying degrees. The descrip- tion that we have just given would not be accurate for all. For the sake of brevity, we have chosen a case which might be that of a goitrous cretin of a certain type, or that of a cretin whose thyroid gland, in lieu of showing what looks like over- growth, has failed to properly develop. Nothing is more remarkable with regard to this organ than the fact that the condition associated with its overgrowth and the effects of its atrophy, or inadequate growth, are the same. A considera- tion of the function of the gland will suggest an explanation of this seeming paradox. The inconvenience caused by goitre induced surgeons, about twenty-five years ago, to remove the tumour in simple un- complicated cases. Owing to the accessibility of the gland, the operation is both safe and easy; but its removal was found to be followed by symptoms of a very serious nature, especially overgrowth and cedema of subcutaneous tissue, muscular twitchings and convulsions, mental dulness. About the same date, physicians recognized that the disease myx- cedema—so called because the cedema is not watery, as in dropsy, but firm and jelly-like—is due to deficiency of the thyroid gland. No other organ of the body has so weird an influence upon the well-being of the whole. No other organ has an equally mysterious ancestral history. Assuredly the thyroid gland was not always such as we see it now. In prevertebrate animals it must have been quite different, both in structure and in function. From fishes upwards, however, its struc- ture is always the same. It is composed of spherical vesicles or globes. Every globe is lined by a single layer of cubical epithelial cells. Its cavity is filled with a homogeneous semi- solid substance known as “‘colloid.”’ The globes are asso- ciated into groups or lobules. They are in contact with large ' , ae de) ry ER KS : Pe aps a = ' £ S = - fe iS aia 3 IX ia ame |); x ~*> uv ~~ S \3 Ge : -g Q KBD, - oo) x < L.) 21/8 ofls & a Fig. 6. The stomach has been cut across a short distance from the pyloric valve, and removed, to show the viscera which lie behind it. The descending aorta and the vena cava rest upon the ver- tebral column. They are crossed by the pancreas and the transverse portion of the duodenum. The head of the pancreas is enclosed by the curvatures of the duodenum. The ducts of the liver and pancreas are seen entering the descending duodenum side by side. cesophagus and stomach is closed by a muscular ring, or sphincter muscle —the cardiac sphincter; the junction of stomach and intestine is guarded by a much stronger pyloric sphincter The average diameter of the small intestine is about 14 inches. It is wide enough, therefore, to admit two fingers. The length of the tube is about 22 feet. Its first part is termed the “ duodenum,” because its length equals the breadth of twelve fingers—7.e., about 9 inches. The remainder is divided aghiteanily into jejunum and mages The 100 = THE BODY AT WORK duodenum makes three sharp curves. First it inclines up- wards and to the right, then vertically downwards, then hori- zontally to the left, and finally forwards. The ducts of the liver and pancreas open by a common orifice into the descending ~ portion. Its horizontal portion is bound firmly to the ver- tebral column. After this the whole of the small intestine is supported by the mesentery, a double fold of peritoneum which allows it to hang freely in the abdominal cavity. The mesen- tery is attached to the back of the body-wall. Commencing on the left side of the second lumbar vertebra, its line of attach- ment inclines obliquely downwards and to the right, across the vertebral column, for about 6 inches. Measured from its attached edge to the edge which bears the intestine, it has a width of about 8inches. Its free border has, as already said, a length of 22feet. Its measurements being as just stated, it is clear that it must be folded backwards and forwards upon itself, like a goffered frill. In the right groin the small intestine joins the large intestine, or colon. It does not, as might have been expected, simply dilate into the large intestine, but enters it on its mesial side, its orifice being guarded by the ileo- colic valve. In other words, the large intestine projects down- wards beyond this orifice, as the czecum coli. In many animals the cecum is of great length and capacity. In the human embryo it begins to assume a similar form ; but a very small portion only (the so-called “cecum” of human anatomy) dilates to the calibre of the colon. The real cecum retains throughout life its embryonic calibre. It has a length of about 34 inches, and a diameter of not more than } inch. This is the “ vermiform appendix,” of ill fame, which must be looked upon as one of Nature’s misfits. Its great liability to become inflamed is commonly explained as due to the tendency of such articles of food as pips, the fibre of ginger, flakes from the inside of enamelled saucepans, etc., to become lodged in its cavity. But whether this explanation be correct or no—and there are reasons for thinking it somewhat fanciful—it is much to be wished that the process of evolution would hasten the dis- appearance of this functionless vestige of a cecum. As there is no tendency towards the inheritance of characters due to mutilation, and since the surgeon’s knife now prevents this death-trap from claiming its toll of possible parents, we must a , DIGESTION: . 101 k ‘upon the paliideaitary cecum, with its liability to inflam- | x ‘mation, as a permanent burden on the human race. In justice to the appendix, however, it must be pointed out that it has acquired its criminal reputation during the past twenty years. The frequency of appendicitis has increased so enormously during this period that it ought to be possible to correlate its prevalence with the introduction of the cause upon which it chiefly depends. The colon has a length of about 5 feet. Its greatest width, about 3 inches, is at its commencement, but it is everywhere much wider than the small intestine. Whereas the wall of the small intestine is smooth externally, the wall of the colon is _ gacculated. Three muscular bands constrict it longitudinally ; circular bands at intervals of about 1 inch or 14 inch throw it into pouches. It ascends on the right side, lying far back against the body-wall, to which it is bound by peritoneum, which in this part of its course covers only its anterior surface. Having touched the under side of the liver, it loops forwards and to the left side, crossing the middle line just above the umbilicus. On the extreme left side it touches the spleen, getting very near to the back of the abdominal cavity. It then descends on the left side, again bound to the body-wall by peritoneum, although not so closely as on the right side, until it reaches the inner lip of the crest of the hip-bone. From here onwards the fold of peritoneum which attaches it allows it a free movement. This portion of the large intestine, the sigmoid flexure, may even fall over into the right groin. Lastly it curls backwards into the pelvis, as the rectum. Movement of the contents of the alimentary canal may be favoured by judicious pressure, or massage. From the descrip- tion of the situation of its several parts given above, it will be understood that if the right hand be placed on the abdomen immediately beneath the ribs, with the fingers well round to the left side, the stomach will be covered. Pressure from left to right will tend to drive its contents towards the pyloric valve. The small intestine is so irregular in its course as to preclude the possibility of following it with the hand. Pressure first on one side and then on the other, with a general tendency to work from above downwards, tends to press forward its contents ; but, owing to its circular form and strong muscular 102 «=53©)©) THE BODY AT WORK _ walls, it is not in much need of help. Very different is the position of the large intestine in this respect. Its calibre is much greater, its wall is sacculated, its contents comparatively firm. If the palm of the hand be placed above the right groin and ‘pressure directed upwards, the czecum coli and ascending colon are emptied. If pressure be directed from the extreme right side just below the ribs, across the middle line to the left side, the transverse colon is emptied. The descending colon needs pressure from above downwards on the left side; the sigmoid flexure, pressure above the left groin, downwards, and towards the middle line. : The inner wall of the cesophagus is smooth, save for the — wrinkles into which it is thrown when not distended ; but from the cardiac orifice of the stomach onwards the mucous mem- brane of the alimentary canal exhibits folds and other pro- jections which serve many purposes. They serve to delay the food, keeping it longer in contact with the secreting surface. They increase the area pitted with tubular glands ; they increase also the area through which absorption of the products of digestion occurs. On the inner surface of the stomach the folds produce a reticulated pattern. In the upper portion of the small intestine, especially the duodenum, there are promi- nent transverse shelves (valvule conniventes). No definite folds occur below the upper three-fourths of the small intestine, with the exception of the constrictions of the transverse colon already referred to, which affect the whole thickness of its wall. Throughout the whole of the small intestine the mucous mem- brane projects in finger-like processes, or villi, which give it a characteristic velvety appearance. The villi are longest in the duodenum. Lympbh-follicles occur at intervals in the intestine. In the ileum they are collected into patches (Peyer’s patches), on the side opposite to the line of attachment of the mesentery. They serve both for the supply of phagocytes, which hunt any germs that have penetrated the mucous membrane, and also as stations to which germ-laden phagocytes retreat. The wall of the intestine is composed of mucous membrane, submucous tissue, and muscle. The mucous membrane is everywhere pitted with tubular glands, termed in the stomach ‘* gastric glands,” and in the intestines, both small and large, erypts of Lieberkiihn.” Their relation to the wall might be exemplified by taking a block of dough about 6 inches thick and pushing a pencil vertically into it almost down to the table on which it rests. The holes should be made as close together as possible, since, especially in the stomach, extremely little tissue intervenes between the tubes of gland-cells. If the piece of dough were placed upon a folded cloth, the cloth would represent the muscularis mucose, a layer properly regarded as a constituent of the mucous membrane. The fibres of this coat are disposed in two or three sheets, the fibres of one sheet crossing those of the next. By their con- tractions they squeeze the ends of the crypts, and probably wobble them about, expelling their secretion. Beneath the muscularis mucose is a layer of connective tissue, the cub- mucosa, which contains abundant lymphatic channels, blood- vessels, and nerves. At the pyloric end of the stomach, the tubes of gland-cells tend to pierce the muscularis mucose. In the first part of the duodenum, certain tubes, having pierced this layer, branch in the submucosa. A layer of racemose glands is thus formed—the glands of Brunner. Outside the sub- mucosa is the muscular coat proper, composed of plain muscle- fibres, except in the upper part of the cesophagus, where the fibres are striated. It consists of an inner and an outer sheet, the fibres being disposed circularly in the inner, longitudinally in the outer sheet, with a slight departure from this regular arrangement in the wall of the stomach. On its outside the canal is invested by peritoneum, a layer of flattened epithelial cells supported by connective tissue. The abdominal wall also is lined with peritoneum. The smooth moist surface of the peritoneum covering the intestines glides on the peritoneum lining the abdominal wall. Between the two is a “ potential ”’ space. In dropsy, fluid accumulates within this space. In a healthy condition the apposed surfaces are merely moist. The movements of the intestines are of two kinds. At all times they exhibit swaying movements, in the production of which the longitudinal fibres play the chief part, although the circular fibres also contract. The object of this undulation is to thoroughly mix the contents of the gut with its secretions. If pills of subnitrate of bismuth are administered, and their progress observed by the aid of Rontgen rays, they are seen to 5 4 ee 104 THE BODY AT WORK oscillate backwards and forwards on their way down the canal. The slower vermicular movement which squeezes the contents forwards is called “ peristalsis.”’ It resembles the pro- gressive contraction of an elastic tube which may be effected by drawing it through a ring, but is rather more complicated. At the point at which it is occurring the circular coat is sharply contracted. Above this it is also somewhat contracted ; below it is relaxed. The longitudinal fibres, using the con- stricted portion as a point d’apput, pull up the segment of the intestine which lies immediately below it, drawing it off the contents of the tube as a glove from a finger. When food is swallowed, it falls down the esophagus, aided by slight peristalsis. As soon as sufficient has accumulated on the upper. surface of the cardiac valve of the stomach, the valve relaxes; at the same time a stronger peristalsis of the lower portion of the cesophagus squeezes its contents into the stomach. Food remains in the stomach until it has reached a certain stage of digestion, the chief object of which is its subdivision into small particles. Until this stage is reached, the pyloric valve is firmly closed. The contractions of the wall of the stomach drive its contents round and round—down the greater and up the lesser curvature—mixing them thoroughly with the gastric juice (cf. p. 124). As the acidity of the mixture increases, the peristaltic contractions of the stomach become more vigorous, until, the pyloric valve relaxing, the food is little by little driven into the duodenum. The alimentary canal has an abundant supply of nerves from the vagus and the sympathetic systems. It contains also within its own wall an enormous quantity of nerve-fibres and nerve- cells. They are disposed as two plexuses, one in the sub- mucosa, the other between the circular and longitudinal muscular coats. In a specimen successfully stained with methylene blue, they are so abundant as to give the impression that every plain muscle-cell may have its own separate nerve- twig. Nevertheless, the contraction of the muscle-cells may take place independently of all nerve-influence—independently, even, of the local mechanism, the plexus referred to above. Nicotin applied to the wall of the intestine paralyses the local nerves ; yet rhythmic contractions still occur. They are, how- ever, no longer progressive. They do not drive the contents Sr, = ‘DIGESTION eT as 105 F ; of the intestine forwards. Co-ordinated contraction is observed so long as the local mechanism is intact, even though all ae. external nerves have been cut. The intestines have their own nerve cells and fibres, which, acting as a linked system of reflex centres, provide for the harmonious contraction of their walls. External nerves, sympathetic and splanchnic, convey impulses which either intensify the movements or inhibit them, as need may be. In the matter ofiits nerve-supply, the alimentary canal stands apart from the other organs of the body. It may be supposed that it presents a more primitive condition. Its muscular fibres have the power of contracting spontaneously. The pressure of the contents of the tube acts as a stimulus. When the fibres are stretched, they contract. When the tube is dilated, its muscles endeavour to restore it to its normal calibre. Such direct action would not, however, provide for the forward passage of its contents. To bring about peristalsis, a nervous mechanism is needed, as abundant and complicated as that which ensures the progress of a slug ora worm. ‘To deal satis- factorily with the various contents of the tube—liquid, solid, gaseous—the mechanism must be capable of complicated ad- justments. The dilated portions of the tube—stomach, cecum coli, rectum—require special arrangements of muscle and nerve. Nor is the canal altogether independent of the rest of the body. To a large extent its work is carried on without regard to the activities of other organs, yet it is not wholly free from the control of the central nervous system. It is regulated by means of both afferent and efferent nerves of the vagus and sympa- thetic. Even the brain has something to say with regard to the way in which it shall contract. It is a matter of common experience that emotional influences may affect the movements of the stomach and intestines—‘“ His bowels yearned.” Normally, vomiting is due to irritation of the endings of the vagus nerve in the stomach, although the afferent impulses may have other sources. Touching the upper surface of the epiglottis with the finger will provoke the reflex. So also will stimulation of the olfactory nerves by a foul smell. In this latter case the emotion of disgust to which the odour gives rise brings about the reflex action. A flow of saliva precedes the act of vomiting. A deep inspiration is then taken, in order that for a time the en may be independent ofa fres! ofair. The glottis is closed, the diaphragm fixed. Contracti of the abdominal wall presses the stomach against the di phragm ; its cardiac sphincter relaxes, and its contents are squirted into the oesophagus, which undergoes a forcible retrogressive peristalsis. It is interesting to note the difference between carnivora and — “a herbivora in regard to vomiting. Carnivora swallow fur and other indigestible materials, as well as many unwholesome things which they need to be able to return. A dog can, apparently, vomit at will. Never, while in a state of nature, do herbivora need to return the contents of the stomach. No provision is made for vomiting. A heifer which has strayed into a dewy clover-field is not unlikely to die from the effects of distension of its paunch, if relief be not given by opening it with a knife. In a horse the cardiac sphincter is strong, the pyloric weak. Pressure on the stomach tends to drive its contents through the pyloric valve into the duodenum, not backwards into the esophagus. 'The stomach is not so placed as to allow of its being compressed between the wall of the abdomen and the diaphragm. Horses cannot vomit. It is a mistake to suppose that they suffer from sea-sickness. In rough weather they sweat, their limbs tremble, they go off their feed; but these symptoms are probably due to the fatigue which results from excessive anxiety to maintain their balance, and to fear. We can never know their feelings, but there is no reason for supposing that they experience the sensation of nausea. Vomiting is a frequent symptom of cerebral disturbance. The fluctuations of pressure which the brain experiences as it rocks ~ about on its ‘“‘ water-bed ” within the skull is the cause of sea- sickness. Yet the motion of a ship may produce violent headache without nausea, the brain only, not the stomach, appearing to be troubled by the motion. Not that headache is a pain “inside the head.” Nor is it properly described as a pain in the scalp, although the messages which are felt in consciousness as headache originate in the endings of the nerves of the skin which covers the skull. The excessive sensitiveness of these nerves is due to vaso-motor conditions, usually the dilation, occasionally the constriction, of the oe a © oe DIGESTION 107 : ploodvessels of the scalp. But the vaso-motor condition is _ sympathetic with the disturbance of the brain ; and the special urgency or efficiency of the messages from the skin results from their being delivered into excited brain-tissue. Nausea and headache are equally symptoms of the irritability of the brain caused by the motion of the ship. In one case messages from the stomach, in the other case messages from the scalp, acquire undue importance, owing to the agitated condition of the brain-tissue through which they pass. Not uncommonly the voyager, who wakes in the morning reconciled to the changes of pressure which he has experienced while recumbent, finds, when he stands upright, that the base of his brain is as sensitive as ever. Visual sensations also contribute to the brain-disturbance. So, too, do the movements of endolymph in the semicircular canals (cf. p. 335). It is, indeed, possible that this last factor is more important than the variations in pressure on the surface of the brain. Probably it accounts for the after-image of rolling which almost everyone experiences for at least a day after leaving the ship. Its cause being cerebral, the tendency to sea-sickness can be controlled by drugs which, like the bromides, chloral, alcohol, etc., deaden the brain. Salivary Glands.—The secretion which accumulates in the mouth is the combined product of the sublingual, sub- maxillary, and parotid glands. It is a very thin, watery solution containing not more than 0-5 per cent. of solid sub- stance. If red litmus-paper is moistened with saliva, it becomes blue, showing that the secretion is alkaline. It contains a ferment, ptyalin, which digests starch. The action of this ferment can be demonstrated by holding in the mouth for half a minute some warm starch mucilage—boiled arrowroot, for example. It quickly loses its viscidity owing to the conversion of starch into sugar. Chemically this change may be demon- strated by adding iodine-water to a specimen of the starch before and after action. Before the starch is taken into the mouth the iodine turns it blue (a characteristic reaction for starch). After it has been exposed to the digestive action of the saliva, iodine fails to colour the mixture, which now contains no starch. All the starch has been converted into dextrin and sugar. If unboiled arrowroot is placed in the mouth, some sugar is eroded: but the process er convantol i slow. It is almost impossible to digest raw starch in t! mouth sufficiently to render it insusceptible to the colouri action of iodine. The sugar produced by the action of ptyalin Be? is of the same nature as that which appears during the malting — of barley. It is therefore termed “maltose.” It closely re- sembles grape-sugar, but is not identical with it. _ The Secretion of Saliva.—The accessibility of the salivary glands, and especially of the submaxillary, has led to their being used for a very large number of experiments. They have been studied with the aim of coming to an understanding of the mechanism of secretion in general. The glands consist of tubes of gland-cells, each tube suspended in a basket of connective tissue, in a bath of lymph (cf. Fig. 3). Innumerable capillary bloodvessels traverse the lymph-bath. The arteries which carry blood to the gland are supplied with nerves, which regulate their calibre, and therefore determine the amount of blood which passes through the capillaries into which they break up. The glands also are supplied with nerves which influence their functional activity. Nutrient substances and oxygen pass out of the blood into the lymph. Carbonic acid passes into the blood from the lymph. Waste products are either carried away in the lymph-stream, or make their way through the walls of the capillaries into the blood. Many problems present themselves for solution. How does the amount of work done by the gland affect its supply of blood ? Does the quantity of saliva secreted vary directly with the pressure of lymph in the spaces by which the gland is surrounded ? Is this pressure wholly dependent upon the pressure of the blood? Are the sub- stances secreted by the gland supplied as such by the blood, or does the gland make the ptyalin and mucus which it secretes? If it makes its secernable products, what materials does it abstract from the blood for the purpose of their manufacture ? Does it use the whole of these materials, whatever they may be, or does it use part only and return the residue to the lymph ? Does it make its products only when it is actively secreting, or is it always making them, and storing them in its cells in order that it may have a supply to discharge when called upon by the stimulation which results from the presence of food in the mouth ? Is their discharge merely a washing-out Ce ee oS ee hy _ - DIGESTION | 109 > to the rush of fluid which occurs when the bloodvessels are dilated, or can the gland-cells expel their products in response - to nervous action ? In what way do the nerves of the gland influence secretion ? Do they call for increased production, or increased output, or both ? These are some of the problems which the exposed situation of the submaxillary gland allows physiologists to tackle. By means of a very simple operation, the ducts of one or both parotid or submaxillary glands can be brought to the skin, and made to pour their secretions on to the surface instead of into the mouth. The flow under various circumstances can be watched. The saliva can be collected and measured. The nerves of the submaxillary gland are easily isolated. A nerve leaves the seventh (or facial), crosses the drum of the ear, comes out through a minute crevice in the skull, and runs for some little distance as a separate nerve before it applies itself to the lingual branch of the fifth, which runs along the side of the tongue. Owing to its passage across the tympanic cavity (drum of the ear), it is termed “‘ chorda tympani.” As its fibres are very small, they can be recognized wherever they form a part of the lingual nerve. They leave the lingual to go to a ganglion, the submaxillary ganglion, from which the gland is supplied. The gland also receives branches from the sympathetic nerve which ascends the neck. The last- named branches accompany the facial artery. Stimulation of either of these nerves causes the gland to secrete. The flow of saliva which follows stimulation of the chorda tympani is much more copious than that which follows stimulation of the sym- pathetic, and as a rule it contains far less organic matter, although about the same amount of mineral salts. Under normal conditions the activity of the chorda tympani is brought into play in a reflex manner by impulses which travel up the nerves of taste (the lingual and glosso-pharyngeal) to the cerebro-spinal axis ; but almost any other nerve will serve as an afferent path. The gland may also, as we shall presently explain, be called into activity by the cortex of the brain. It is certain that in the case of the submaxillary gland secretion is not the direct result of increased blood-pressure. It is not a case of filtration from the blood through certain membranes and cells into the salivary duct. Atropin (bella- = =. - wt a 9.2.) 5 ee LL ee IE Pe re ee eo Rae te Sr er, LS ye 4 or ee hae ; itn: THE BODY AT WORK | donna) dilates the blaodveusele increasing blond presente but a it stops secretion. After belladonna-poisoning, the mouth, like __ the skin, is hot and dry. Other drugs there are which provoke a certain amount of secretion, even after the bloodvessels going to the gland have been tied. It is possible, by stimulating the chorda tympani, to obtain a pressure in the fluid in the duct very much greater than that in the bloodvessels which supply the gland. Here we have clear proof that secretion is not filtration. Filtration is the passage of fluid through a filter-bed from a higher to a lower pressure. In filtration, moreover, soluble diffusible salts accompany the water. The saliva contains only half as much of these diffusible salts as the blood. Therefore the gland tissue stops half the salts. Secre- tion is an active process carried out by the gland-cells, under the influence of nerves, in opposition to the laws of filtration. The gland-cells determine how much water shall pass through them and what percentage of salts shall accompany the water. How does a gland-cell make the substance which it secretes ? There is no reason for supposing that the ptyalin or the mucus which the salivary glands secrete is present in the blood, either ready formed, or, as it were, half formed, in combinations which can be easily broken up. All the evidence obtainable points to the conclusion that the gland-cells take out of the lymph proteid materials from which they manufacture the peculiar substances which they secrete. During rest, granules accumulate in the cells. During activity they disappear. It has been shown in the case of the gastric glands that these granules consist of the special ferment which the gland secretes, in an inactive form. It may be that it is combined with a sub- stance which prevents it from exerting its digestive action on the cells within which it is made; damped, as gunpowder is damped during transit. Or it may be that it is not a finished ferment ; it may need a further addition to its molecule. During activity, while the granules disappear, proteins accumulate at the bases of the cells, giving to a tube of gland-cells the appear- ance of a peripheral non-granular zone. This proteid sub- stance must have come from the lymph, and the inference seems inevitable that the cells have taken into their protoplasm a supply of material which will serve for the manufacture of additional granules. Hach gland-cell is therefore an indepen- , out of which it Gateteoviies its own ibe products. stores its products until they are wanted. Then by its own activity it extrudes them into the lumen of the gland-tube. It has, indeed, been shown that, when the nerve going to a salivary gland is stimulated, the gland shrinks, notwithstanding the great dilation of its bloodvessels. Under the influence of the stimulation the granules in the gland-cells imbibe water, swell up, and escape from the cells. The cells discharge their accumulated stores, in the first instance, more rapidly than they take up materials (even fluid) from the blood. For its know- ledge (if the term may pass) of what is wanted the gland-cell is _ dependent upon messages which reach it through the nervous system. ‘T’hese messages take origin in the endings of the sensory nerves of the mouth, pass up to the brain, and are reflected down the nerves to the gland. So accurate is the information conveyed to the glands, that when a horse transfers the work of mastication from one side of its mouth to the other, as it is in the habit of doing about every quarter of an hour, the flow of saliva from the parotid gland on the masticating side is increased; on the other side it is diminished. Two or three times as much saliva is poured out on the one side as on the other. Not only is the amount of saliva poured out in response to stimulation proportional to the needs of mastication, but the kind of saliva is adapted to the nature of the food. In a dog— and this is an observation which can be made only on an animal which lives on a mixed diet—it is possible to determine the amount of the two kinds of saliva secreted and the relation of flow to food. When meat is given to the animal, the submaxillary gland yields its secretion; when it is fed on biscuit, abundance of the watery aa saliva is poured forth. A mouthful of sand also causes the parotid saliva to flow, in order that the sand may be washed out of the mouth. More remarkable than the response to direct stimulation is the effect produced by the sight and smell of food. When meat is shown to a dog, submaxillary saliva begins to flow; when it is offered bread, parotid saliva is secreted. And the activity of the glands is not merely a nervous reflex independent of the animal’s mind. The moment the dog realizes that it is being played with—that there is no intention of giving it the coveted 4 = Be Ts ees: when every physiological condition is demanding it. This is memnicreafitoas he ~ , =, o> 7h ae es) “oe oe bt ey Me THE ‘BODY AT <* tg food—the flow of saliva ceases. An emotion may check secretion the explanation of the Rice Ordeal. Dry rice provokes a flow of saliva in the mouth of all save the guilty man. Response to mental impressions is a matter of the greatest consequence ~ in the physiology of digestion. It holds good in the case of the secretion of gastric juice equally with that of saliva. The sight and smell of food sets the juice flowing into the stomach, and the more desirable the food, the more attractive its appear- ance, the more stimulating its smell, the more rapidly does the secretion flow. Here we touch upon a theme which hardly needs exhaustive treatment. It is not the stoutest people who eat the most, although an impartial survey of one’s well- - nourished friends will show them to be persons who “take kindly ' to their victuals.” A small quantity of food perfectly digested _ is more nourishing than much food which the digestive organs do not efficiently prepare for assimilation. Good digestion waits on appetite ; and appetite, in civilized man, is something more than a mere physical need of food. The hunger which leads to the bolting of food without pleasurable anticipation, without mastication, without any consideration of the quality of the viands, is a harmful craving which ends in imperfect assimila- tion. It is more profitable to toy with a hors duvre than to engulf, unthinking, a plateful of beef. But we have said enough to suggest reflections to those who take no thought as to what they shall eat or what they shall drink ; and few who take thought need to be convinced. The Stomach.—The sight and smell of food, its presence in the mouth, and the performance of mastication, which induces a secretion of saliva, gives rise at the same time to a flow of gastric juice. It is psychic stimulation and the act of eating which cause gastric juice to ooze from the gland-tubes of the stomach at the commencement of digestion, not the stimulation of nerve-endings by food which has passed down the cesophagus. As a consequence of gunshot wounds, or as the result of operations performed for the purpose of relieving patients whose cesophagus has become blocked, numerous cases have been recorded in which a fistulous opening into the stomach has made it possible to study the interior of this organ. Such cases present an opportunity of PAMEIEN AG NED COG “TORONTO URTV. | a) ae ig the digestion of various foods introduced through the ening, and of collecting gastric juice for purposes of analysis. _ Asimilar condition has been established in animals by operative means. The cesophagus having been cut, and the cut end sutured to the margins of an aperture in the skin, food taken by _ the mouth escaped by this opening instead of passing into the stomach. A similar opening was made into the stomach for the insertion of food, and for the purpose of studying the effects of reflex stimulation of the gastric glands. As soon as food was introduced into the mouth, gastric juice began to flow. The advantage of this experimental method lies in the fact that the juice secreted was a pure juice—not mixed with food, as in all the earlier experiments in which, the stomach being _ opened without diversion of the cesophagus, the presence of food within it was the stimulus which led to secretion. No juice flowed in the absence of stimulation; nor was the secre- tion normal in composition when provoked by a mechanical stimulus, such as the tickling of the gastric mucous membrane by a feather. My lord the stomach! He is not the only, nor is he the chief, agent in digestion; but with him rests the decision as to whether the food offered to the alimentary tract is suitable in quality and quantity. He is offended if it be not offered with all the circumstance and ceremony which becomes his rank. As an intimation that he is about to receive food, he accepts the news from the mouth that its nerve-endings are subject to mechanical stimulation. But the chewing of india- rubber would produce a like effect. The stomach, therefore, confers with the organs of taste and smell. If their report is favourable, he argues that the substance which the teeth are crushing will justify an outflow of gastric juice. He responds most generously when prolonged mastication assures him that he may trust to receiving the food in a sufficiently subdivided state. At our peril we neglect to propitiate my lord. Not always debonair when treated with consideration, he is morose or petulant when slighted. Never content with lip- service, he exacts the labour of teeth and tongue and palate. _ The tribute we offer may be of the best—savoury, wholesome, well cooked, well chewed—but if it be not tendered with some degree of love, if thoughts are concentrated on other things, P 8 > as ite a i ee ~ = nee 2 tae as all Mun By th ifn no attention is devoted’ to the wal if ibaeleee ikin panies our offering, my lord the stomach « on his part affords viands an indifferent reception. In consulting our own tas we are to a large extent consulting the needs of the stom AC. ae Ravenous and excessive feeding is not an exhibition of taste ; it is a return to the instinct of the savage, who was never sure q that he would get his full share, and was afraid to trust that i. another meal would be obtainable when nature declared it due. Some degree of epicureanism is favourable to digestion. The flow of gastric juice in the stomach occurs reflexly in response to the emotion of appetite, to stimulation of the nerves of taste and smell, to the obscure sensations which accompany the activity of the muscles of mastication. The gastric juice secreted in a day amounts probably to about 8 or 9 pints. To this we must add, when considering the quantity of fluid which passes through the stomach, the saliva, which certainly reaches as much as 2 pints, and the beverages taken with food. . |. Gastric juice collected in the manner described above is a f 6, clear, colourless, inodorous fluid. It is very acid, and so power- \w fully peptic as to digest its own weight of coagulated white / | of egg. Its solid constituents amount_to 0-5 per cent. They “\ | consist of the two ferments pepsin and rennin, with traces of ‘ proteins and mucin, and various inorganic salts. Its acidity f is due to free hydrochloric acid to the amount of 0-2 per cent. This acid is more or less in combination with the pepsin. In pure gastric juice hydrochloric acid is the only acid present ; | but when mixed with food the juice contains other acids also, especially lactic. When food first reaches the stomach, the alkaline saliva | which accompanies it neutralizes the acidity of the gastric | juice. For some time, probably about half an hour, the conversion of starch into sugar is still carried on by the ptyalin | of the saliva, owing chiefly to the difficulty which the gastric | juice encounters in permeating the masses of masticated food. The Bacillus acids lactict is always present in the stomach. It converts some of the sugar into lactic acid; of this a small quantity is further changed into but butyric and acetic acids, with the formation of carbonic acid and h: hydrogen gas. ‘After a a while the lactic acid is absorbed, and hydrochloric acid alone remains. mm F Ly oF; ‘ oe (i - *, at We tale . a 4 le ples of 80 argh a eedecat ssi. oe plant -cells to produce it without injury to the , or for the stomach to contain it without self-digestion ? By chemical and physical theories have been advanced in B eliet that they rendered the process of its production less 4 if cult to understand. All such theories are, however, i 23 _ adequate to explain the secretion as a discontinuous process, which occurs only as a response to demand. That the source _ of the acid is the sodic chloride which the gland-cells take from, / ydroe’ ates always aroused interest. How is it ‘ ty the blood does not need assertion, but we cannot picture ‘the of process by which this exceedingly stable compound is decom- _ “posed otherwise than on the assumption that weaker acids, or, rather, acid salts, are also absorbed by the cells, and that, in accordance with the laws which govern the composition of salts in solution, an exchange of acids occurs. If sodic chloride and any acid salt—acid phosphate of sodium, for example— are in solution in water, the salts do not retain their form as we know them when isolated by crystallization. The mixture contains “free”? hydrochloric as well as “free” phosphoric acid. It may be assumed that within secreting cells a similar exchange of acids takes place. By a process which we term “vital,” the acids are kept apart, and the hydrochloric acid is extruded by the cells. In the present state of knowledge this vital action is mysterious ; but it is no more mysterious than the isolation of pepsin, or any other metabolic event which occurs within a cell. The proteolytic ferment pepsin is active only in an acid medium. Yet apart from its digestive function as an ally of pepsin, hydrochloric acid by itself also exerts a valuable disintegrating action on certain constituents of the food. Possibly the most important results of the presence of free hydrochloric acid in the great chamber into which food is first received are due to its disinfective property. It destroys all the putrefactive germs which accompany the food, and many germs which, if introduced into the blood, would give rise to disease. It also destroys the germs which multiply in the stomach towards the end of each interval between two meals. When withdrawn from the body, gastric juice will keep an indefinite time, if evaporation of the acid be prevented. 8—2 resemblance to the salivary pers) "Probably. “’ i blance is merely superficial. Minute examination — points, apparently of great morphological importance, in wk they differ. In the gland-tubes of the salivary glands, and, — indeed, in all glands with the exception of the pancreas, us secreting cells project into the lumen. The secreting cells of the pancreas are invested internally by a layer of — flattened scales .(intra-acinar cells). They lie, therefore, between the basement membrane which invests them exter- nally and this second layer of flattened cells which separates them from the lumen of the tube. At a very early date in em- bryonic life the gland-cells of the pancreas are filled with highly refracting granules. As this occurs long before any digestive action is called for, it may be taken as indicating that the pancreas has functions which other glands—the salivary, for example—do not possess. These granules do not, however, appear in all parts of the tubes. Certain portions of the tubes remain undeveloped—fail, that is to say, to acquire a secreting function—even in adult life. Such patches of cells, not dis- posed in gland-tubes, are known as islands of Langerhans. When the pancreas is over-stimulated by artificial means, leading to its extreme exhaustion, large portions of its glandular substance return to this primitive condition. The gland-cells not only discharge their stores of granules, but they lose the greater part of their cell-protoplasm. It would seem that, in their effort to meet the demand for ferments,. they use up their own cell-substance in their manufacture. Having exhausted their coal, they stoke the furnace with the looms and furniture of the mill. It may be that other glands would do the same if it were possible to stimulate them as strongly as the pancreas can be stimulated. The result is probably due to the extreme susceptibility of the pancreas to the action of secretin, a substance made in the intestine. Secretin can be isolated and injected into the blood. We shall refer again to this chemical stimulation of the pancreas when tracing the progress of food through the alimentary canal. The secretion of the pancreas is a clear, colourless, alkaline liquid of syrupy consistence. The quantity of juice secreted is usu smal but the organic substances which it contains ieee 117. in a concen ia form. They constitute as much as 10 per of the pancreatic juice. Proteins are present, if the F juice be fresh. If it has stood for any length of time, they are - found as peptones. The digestive ferments of pancreatic juice 4 are the most powerful which are seareted into the alimentary canal. ~ Bile.—In its most important functions the liver has no rela- tion to digestion. It is a storehouse of absorbed food. This organ will therefore be treated in a separate chapter. The bile which the liver secretes into the alimentary canal has no _ ene chemical action on any of the constituents of food, with the exception of a feeble tendency to digest starch. Yet it is in some degree accessory to digestion. Poured into the second portion of the duodenum through an orifice common to the liver and the pancreas, it mingles with the semi-digested food, or “ chyme,”’ which, about two hours after a meal, passes through the pyloric valve. Gastric digestion has converted the greater part of the proteid constituents of the food into peptones or intermediate stages. ‘The proteoses or propeptones—a name is needed for the intermediate products of proteid digestion which does not commit us to any theory as to their chemical con- stitution—are quickly peptonized by the pancreatic juice. But portions of the proteins have escaped the action of gastric juice, or have at most been affected by its acid only ; these are precipitated by the bile-salts on the mucous membrane of the small intestine, which is raised into projecting flanges for _ the purpose of delaying the passage of the chyme, in order that it may be thoroughly submitted to the digestive action of pan- creatic juice. Bile-salts also favour the digestion of fat, and its passage through the intestinal wall. The action of bile- salts in spreading fats is well known to artists. Ox-gall is smeared upon glass when it is desired to apply oil-paints to its surface. When mixed with oil, it causes its emulsification, or breaking up into microscopic globules. In the absence of bile, but little fat passes into the lymph-vessels which convey digested food from the intestine to the thoracic duct, and so to the great veins of the neck. Its action is mechanical. It favours the digestion of fats by rendering them easily amenable to hydrolysis by pancreatic juice. Bile as secreted by the liver is a clear, limpid fluid of low a ee se we feed Ce ee oe j BTS ee nse eee NO BET eh Polar oa ee 411g “THE BODY AT WORK specific gravity ; but during its stay in the , ponoentraed by absorption-of water, and mucin is added tots a t contains “ bile-salts ” of complex constitution. These salts _ favour the solution of certain by-products of cell-metabolism, cholesterin and lecithin; substances which are formed in =) many célls, both in animals and plants. Cholesterin occurs most abundantly in nerve-tissue and in blood-corpuscles. Lecithin also is a by-product of the metabolism of nerve-tissue. Protoplasm appears to be incapable of oxidizing these sub- stances, as it does other products of metabolism. Other sub- stances of equally complex constitution are reduced to urea if they contain nitrogen; to water and carbonic acid if nitrogen be absent. Cholesterin and lecithin have to be eliminated without further change. Some of the cholesterin is excreted by the sebaceous glands of the skin. It is the chief constituent — of “lanoline”’ prepared from sheep’s wool; an unguent which owes its valuable properties to the resistance which cholesterin offers to cell action, and therefore to the action of living fer- ments. Bacteria ‘cannot turn it rancid. The sebaceous glands have the power of directing metabolism into a channel in which cholesterin is the chief product, but apparently all cells make it in small quantity. The bile-salts carry choles-~ ‘terin and lecithin into the alimentary canal, from which they are not reabsorbed. Some of the bile-salts are lost to the body, but the remainder re-enter the circulation, and recommence their work as vehicles for these inoxidizable and insoluble substances. In the gall-bladder cholesterin is apt to separate out from the bile in the form of gall-stones ; but whether this is due to an excess of cholesterin in the bile, or to an abnormal, inflammatory condition of the lining membrane of the gall- bladder, is still an open question. Bile also contains bile-pigments. Their colour varies in different animals, and changes according as the bile is exposed to the air, or subject to the action of reducing agents. If oxidized, the colour is green (biliverdin) ; if reduced, brownish- yellow (bilirubin). Bile- -pigment is formed from hemoglobin, the colouring matter of the blood, after the removal of its iron. Worn-out red blood-corpuscles are destroyed in the spleen, in the manner already described, but it is uncertain whether the conversion of the hemoglobin thus set free into bilirubin brane of the alimentary ract bie pay as the middle of the rectum, is, as previously d (p. 102), studded cadres a y secrete a Its 1t-yellow fluid, alkaline in reaction, and opalescent. line to Ne aa sueage the liver we 20st important property is due to a ferment which converts ‘ eamie-Sugar~into a mixture of dextrose and levulose, and i oem maltose—the sugar produced by the action on starch of saliva and pancreatic juice—into dextrose. It is in the form of dextrose that sugar is carried about the body and assimilated by the tissues. ‘ Intestinal juice also contains a ferment, erepsin, which shakes to pieces the heavy molecules of peptones and partly formed -_peptones. Under its influence they break up into compara- tively simple bodies containing the radicle of ammonia. Substances containing an NH, group—one H of NH, (ammonia) having been given up, in order that the group may have a ** free arm ”’ with which to link on to the other component parts of the molecule—are termed “‘ amides.” The amides which are most characteristic of the action of erepsin are leucin, an amidated fatty acid ; and tyrosin, an amidated aromatic acid. The tendency of proteins to break up along these two lines— the fatty acid line and the aromatic acid line—is of consider- able interest. The one line is represented by acetic acid, CH,,COOH ; the other contains the hexone radicle, C,H,. Ben- zoic acid, C,H,COOH, is representative of the latter. It used to be thought that proteins which were shaken into simple bodies such as amides were lost to the economy. Their downward career was a foregone conclusion. There could be no arresting it before they brought up at the bottom—as urea, CO(NH,),.— the diamide of carbonic acid. It was even supposed that this disintegration of proteins was a provision for getting rid of .the surplus animal food which we consume. Physiological chemists now take quite a different view. They believe that the epithelial wall of the intestine through which these sub- stances are absorbed, or the liver, to which they are carried by the portal blood-stream, has the power of recombining these fragments into the complex protein edifice. It is even "420 ‘THE BODY AT WORK _ thought that disintegration is a necessary preliminary to the —__ rearrangement of the sub-groups. A large variety of proteins is ingested as food. Many of them, especially the vegetable proteins, are quite foreign to the body. By the activity of pancreatic juice and erepsin, they are broken into small and relatively stable groups of atoms, which are again fitted together into the particular forms of protein which are of use to the economy. The Story of a Meal.—The chemistry of digestion will be understood most readily if the constituents of a meal are traced from their entrance into the mouth to their absorption through the wall of the alimentary canal, or abandonment as indigestible. ; We may describe as a typical meal one consisting of bread, vegetables, cane-sugar, meat, milk, fat, and cheese. In the mouth the various foods are crushed and mixed with the alka- line secretions of the salivary glands. A certain amount of the cooked starch contained in the bread is changed into maltose. In the stomach the digestion of starch is continued for a time, but a large part even of the cooked starch awaits the action of pancreatic juice. A certain amount of cane-sugar is converted into dextrose and levulose, which are rapidly absorbed into the blood ; but this action is due to hydrochloric acid, and probably affects a comparatively small part of the cane-sugar swallowed. Fat is quite unaltered in the stomach. All proteins are attacked by pepsin, but some yield to digestion more readily than others, Gluten of bread, like all vegetable proteins, is comparatively resistant ; but since it is presented to the action of pepsin in small quantities and in a spongy form—very suitable for digestion—it is probable that most of it is peptonized in the stomach. Chemists experimenting with gastric juice taken from the stomach, and reproducing the conditions as to temperature, removal of products of action, etc., as closely as it is possible to reproduce them in the labora- tory, find that the various foods take different times to digest. The proteins of meat are more quickly peptonized when raw than after coagulation by heat. The same is true of white of egg. Amongst different varieties of cooked flesh, beef is more quickly peptonized than fish. The casein of milk is more quickly peptonized than any other protein ; and it also is no ; tion “a a4 ale that digestibility i is diminished by cooking. Similar data may be obtained for all foods. They are no doubt useful indications of the course of action which we may expect __ to occur within the stomach, but we can never be sure that my lord will obey the ruling of the chemist. Practice with a __ captive golf-ball is a useful preparation for the game ; but there are conditions on the links which cannot be reproduced on the lawn. In an artificial stomach the clean fibre of raw fish _ digests more slowly than raw beef. Even when the beef is roasted and the fish fried or boiled in the ordinary way, the beef disappears through the dialyser (the bag of membrane suspended in a vessel of warm water in which experimental digestion is carried out) more quickly than the fish. Never- theless, the living stomach is better disposed towards a mixed meal containing a certain weight of fish than towards a meal in which, the other constituents remaining the same, beef takes the place of fish. Important conclusions may, no doubt, be drawn from observations of the time occupied in the peptoniza- tion of pure food—.e., fibrin, white of egg, clean meat, etc.— under conditions simulating those which are present in the stomach ; but they must be accepted with many reservations. In the a eaark it is not pure substances, but mixtures, that the gastric juice has to deal with. And here a most important factor comes into play, to which further reference will be made later on. The amount and quality of the secretion of the gastric glands depends upon the nature of the food. Hence a food, or a combination of foods, which digest readily in the laboratory may take a long time to disappear from the stomach, and vice versd. NDigestibility depends upon the nature of the food. It depends also upon its physical state. To take simple illustrations : Cheese contains coagulated casein, one of the most easily digestible of proteins, but the casein is intimately mixed with fat, upon which gastric juice can make no impression. Even when finely divided, the particles of casein are protected from the action of the juice by fat. In the same -way the meat of pork is as digestible as mutton, but the fat of pork is quickly melted and very liquid. In the process of cooking the muscle-fibres become saturated with fat. It is not the function of the stomach to complete digestion. Its business is to initiate it. Food which reaches the stomach tion heat a much larger proparian of intonmedians ducts, proteoses or propeptones, than does digestion in t duodenum. Such intermediate products are quickly dealt with by pancreatic juice. Artificial tests of relative igen oe ; bility do not, as a rule, take the amount of propeptones formed in a given time into account. When considering the digestion - of a typical meal, we must bear in mind that it is not the duty of the stomach to pass as much sugar, peptone, and fat as possible into the blood. In fact, very few of the products of digestion are absorbed by th the bloodvessels of the the stomach, The impermeability of its mucous membrane is shown by the fact that hardly any of the water swallowed passes through the stomach-wall. Practically all the water ingested leaves the stomach through the pyloric valve. Various salts, some sugar, and peptones are taken up by the vessels of the stomach ; but the bulk of all the different kinds of food passes. into. the duodenum in a semi-digested state. The function of the stomach is to carry digestion through a preliminary stage. “The process will be completed in the small intestine. It is to be noted that, although water is not absorbed by the stomach- wall, alcohol passes through it with great rapidity. The same is true of the various crystalline nitrogenous bodies found in_ meat-extracts, and also of the essential principles of tea and coffee, which chemically belong to the same class. All these substances are degradation products of proteins produced by oxidation, far advanced along the road to urea. In this selec- tive absorption we see proof of the activity of the cells of the mucous membrane. They take up the substances which it is desirable to remove from the contents of the stomach. Some may be wanted by the body for its immediate use; others are better out of the way, because they are prejudicial to the progress of digestion. When contemplating the activity of the cells of the gastric mucous membrane, we feel the need of an adjective which shall express our recognition of the fact that they have a power which we cannot confer upon our clumsy mechanical imitation stomach. They can_discriminate. “Vital” is the only term available, though much abused. Using it without 3 54 Lars ‘ > oe ee he sane 123 - prejudi o, as lawyers say, we aphak of the “ vital activity ” of the cells when we wish to imply that things happen i in a living _ stomach for which we cannot make provision in a model. Of _ ~ the many substances which make their appearance as digestion __ proceeds, some are absorbed, others left in the mixture. a The mucous membrane shows its powér~ of controlling i digestion in yet another way. In the neighbourhood of the ‘ pylorus its structure is unlike that which it “presents elsewhere. The gastric glands are short, and tend to branch. Their lining cells are all of the same kind. Over the greater part of the inner wall of the stomach the tubes are long. They do not branch. The cells which line them are of two kinds: small cubical cells (the term refers to their form as seen in section), similar to those of the pyloric glands; large oval cells, placed with their longest axes in the same direction as the axis of the gland-tube. These oval cells do not project into the bore or lumen of the tube, but are displaced from it by the cubical cells. They rest on the investing, or basement, membrane. All parts of the gastric mucous membrane secrete pepsin, although the pyloric portion produces very little ; the area which contains oval cells alone secretes hydrochloric acid. If a short time after a meal an extract is made from some of the mucous membrane near the pylorus, by pounding it with salt-solution and sand to break up its cells, this extract, when filtered and injected into the blood, stimulates the glands of the cardiac end of the stomach. Under its influence they pour out both pepsin and hydrochloric acid. The extract contains a substance which acts as a chemical messenger. It is a repre- sentative of a class of bodies which play a most important part in co-ordinating the activities of the various organs. Hitherto physiologists have concerned themselves with the visible or *‘ external ”’ secretions of glands. They have shown how the production of these secretions is controlled by the nervous system. Recently they have discovered that another set of influences has to be taken into consideration. Glands, and possibly all other tissues, take from the blood the materials out of which they make their characteristic secretions, or, if they do not discharge secretions, the substances which they require for the building of their own structures, and return to the blood “ internal secretions ”’ which act as stimuli to other — eee al ie ee 124, | 'THE BODY AT WORK — tissues with which they are linked in harmonious. co-operation, —— The active principles of internal secretions have been termed bend ’—from opudw, I announce. The glands of the pyloric mucous membrane secrete a hormone which calls upon the rest of the membrane to pour out gastric juice (cf. p. 89). What induces the cells of the pyloric mucous membrane to produce the gastric hormone ? Their activity in this respect evidently depends upon the presence in the stomach of par- tially digested proteid substances. ‘The cells judge, as it were, when these substances come into contact with them, that there is more work for the great bag of the stomach to do. They call upon the part which is most active in secreting gastric juice to pour it out quickly and get the business of digestion over. Meat-extracts, which contain the products of protein disintegration, have a similar influence in promoting the forma- tion of the hormone. Hence, no doubt, the general custom, found from experience to be beneficial, of commencing dinner with soup; although it must be remembered that the rapid_ absorption of meat-extracts makes them peculiarly valuable as restoratives. They afford very little energy, but what they | have to give is quickly placed at the disposal of the economy. Persons whose stomachs are unduly irritable are advised to avoid soup. It leads to undesirable activity on the part of the gastric glands, and especially of the acid-secreting cells. Well-chewed bread also encourages the production of the hormone. Here it may be well to call attention to the evident division of the stomach into two parts—the large bag, or cardiac portion, which hangs down; and the smaller, funnel-shaped pyloric end, which is almost vertical. The distinction between these two parts is faintly visible in the resting stomach, but even opening the abdomen tends to obliterate it. That it is much more evident during active digestion has been shown by adding subnitrate of bismuth to the food, and throwing the shadow of the stomach on a screen with Rontgen rays. When this is done, it is seen that the two parts work in different ways. Food is churned round and round in the cardiac portion, and pressed towards the pylorus. Its fluid products, mixed with the abundant secretion of the gastric mucous membrane, are wrung out of it by the pyloric funnel. They are squeezed towards the pylorus, which opens at intervals tolet them through. If lumps of solid matter reach it, the pyloric valve closes tightly, until the undigested food has fallen back into the dependent bag. Dyspeptics are sometimes unpleasantly conscious of the contractions of the pyloric funnel. =, ~ In fact, putting aside pain due to gastritis, all the discomfort of dyspepsia is felt on the right side. Flatus accumulates beneath the pyloric valve. The valve will not open to let it pass. The pyloric portion of the stomach contracts strongly. Notwithstanding the general trend of movement in the opposite direction, the gases are squeezed back into the larger bag, and escape through the cardiac orifice. Tables have been prepared showing the length of time which various articles of food take to digest. They are based in part upon observations made upon the living stomach in cases in which it has been possible to examine its contents through a fistulous opening ; in part upon the results of arti- ficial digestions carried out in the laboratory. It is hardly too much to say that such observations are absolutely without value as tests of the relative digestibility of the several articles of diet consumed as parts of an ordinary meal. The fact that the commencement of the flow of gastric juice depends upon mental stimuli, and its continuance upon hormones, shows how difficult it must be to reproduce the conditions which obtain in a healthy living body. The most wholesome of foods taken by itself may be longer in digesting, or may produce more irritation, than many less desirable things taken in judicious combination. Crushed chicken, hastily swallowed, sometimes proves more difficult of digestion than meat so cooked and served as to stimulate appetite and to demand mastication. Returning to the story of a meal, vegetables pass almost unaltered through the stomach. Some of the scanty proteins which they contain are peptonized, but unless they are very well masticated or cooked until they are soft, and therefore easily pulped by the churning action of the stomach, the gastric juice has to reach the proteins through* cell-walls. None of the digestive juices are able to dissolve the cellulose of vegetable cell-walls. Blocks of vegetable tissue pass down the whole length of the alimentary canal in the form in which they were left by the teeth. Hence the extreme indigestibility of ill-chewed snuibilier or apple. The pyloric Salve af the stotiindl . is forbidden to allow any lumps of food to pass until the very last stage of gastric digestion. Pieces of ill-masticated vege-. table tissue lie for a long time in the stomach, i | of the gastric nerves, until at last the time comes for them to be shot through the pylorus into the duodenum. Many salts which vegetables contain, especially the earthy carbonates | and phosphates, are dissolved by the acid of the gastric juice. Meat consists of muscle-fibres supported by connective tissue. In the stomach the gelatiniferous connective tissue is dissolved, setting the fibres free. Further, the fibres being surrounded by a membrane of the same nature—sarcolemma— this is removed ; and although it may be hardly justifiable to speak of “‘ Krause’s membranes ”’ (cf, Fig. 10) as gelatiniferous septa, the fibres are certainly composed of segments—Bow- man’s discs, sarcous elements—into which they break up under the action of gastric juice. As a result, meat-fibre is reduced to a finely divided granular condition. The capacity of gastric juice for dissolving collagen (the substance of which connective tissue is composed) may be regarded as its most characteristic, as it is one of its most valuable, properties. Collagen, when boiled or acted on~by acids, takes water into its molecule, becoming gelatin. Under the influence of gastric juice gelatin is rapidly hydrolysed into diffusible gelatin-peptone. Pan- creatic juice is unable to act upon collagen, unless it has been previously boiled, or swollen by the action of dilute acids. Fat is composed of vesicles of oil supported by connective tissue. Gastric juice, by dissolving the connective tissue and the collagenous walls of the vesicles, sets the oil free. The oil, even though it be as firm as suet when cold, is liquid, or almost liquid, at the temperature of the body. Thus, with the exception of raw vegetables, the hard fibre of cooked vegetables, elastic tissue of meat, and a few other indigestible substances, the meal is reduced in the stomach to a cream-coloured, fatty, strongly acid “ chyme.” In this condition it enters the duodenum, where it at once comes into contact with an alkaline secretion. The passage of acid chyme down this portion of the canal provokes the discharge of gushes of bile and pancreatic juice. By precipitating partialiy digested proteins and “ acid-albumin ”’ bile renders I actecnee are. of panera converted into peptones, some of ich are shaken down by the violent action of erepsin into - simpler bodies, such as leucin and tyrosin, etc. The chyme rat ee alkaline, grey, and thin. All anidsancked akaeok 7a ia unged into maltose, and this into dextrose. Cane-sugar is converted into dextrose and levulose. These sugars are absorbed into the blood. Milk-sugar, if not_ converted into lactic a acid, remains as lactose (CisH:011), in which condition it is absorbed without “i inversion.” Fats are split by a ferment of the pancreatic. juice into fatty acid and _ glycerin ; some of the fatty acid combines with alkali to form Soak. but of this we shall have more to say later on. he duct common to the liver and the pancreas opens into the second part of the duodenum. The organs which produce bile and pancreatic juice are comparatively remote from the “place where their secretions come into contact with the food. By what mechanism are they thrown into activity when the assistance of their secretions is required ? As in the case of the stomach, the agent is a hormone, a chemical messenger. The hormone, termed “secretin,” is formed by the cells of the Mucous membrane of the duodenum when acid comes in contact with them. It is absorbed by the blood, which carries it to the pancreas and the liver. When it reaches the pancreas, it acts as a most powerful stimulant to the discharge of accumu- lated ferments, and to the production of an additional supply. It stimulates the liver to pour forth bile. At present we are in ignorance as to the chemical nature of this hormone. It is ee substance, nor is it aferment. If scrapings from @ mucous membrane of the duodenum be crushed with sand and hydrochloric acid, the mixture boiled, neutralized with carbonate of soda, and filtered, the clear, colourless liquid which results has a powerful effect upon the pancreas, when injected, in even small quantities, into the blood. Apparently, the cells of the duodenal mucous?membrane are constantly producing and accumulating a substance which is converted into secretin when acted on by acid. It is not necessary for the acid to stimulate the living cells.*} If the mucous membrane is ground up with sand and salt-solution, the filtrate is inactive: re” Fee ee eS ee. oe ee ee eee oi, ae oe on ae x = 128 THE BODY AT WORK but an active extract is obtained by treating the crushed cells with HCl. It changes some substance which they contain (pro- visionally termed “ prosecretin ’’) into the efficient hormone. In the lo ion of the small intestine any maltose that: remains is converted into diffusible dextrose. A very large amount of water has been poured into the canal in the various digestive juices. This, together with water drunk, is absorbed _in the large intestine. At the lower end of the - alimentary canal tr remains but indigestible substances - taken with food, chiefly cellulose, and the pigments and other — bodies which, as already said, are eliminated in bile. The absorption of water is checked by the ingestion of extremely soluble salts, such as sulphate of magnesia, the heavy molecule of which diffuses with difficulty. We attribute the fact that sulphate of magnesia remains in the intestine, and prevents water from diffusing out of it, to its slowness in passing through a membrane, because this is what would happen in dialysis ;* but we must remember that the living wall * Notice the distinction between filtration and dialysis. If water containing soluble and insoluble substances is placed in a porous jar, the water and the soluble substances pass through the pores of the jar. The rate of flow varies as the pressure. If water containing soluble substances is placed in a bladder, and the bladder is suspended in a vessel of water, some of the substances which it may contain—white of egg, for example—are non-diffusible ; others will pass from the water inside the bladder to the water which surrounds it. But every diffusible substance has its own osmotic value, Some pass through the membrane rapidly, soon establishing a condition of equilibrium in the two fluids; others take a long time. Further, if the water on one side of the membrane contains a certain percentage weight of a salt, the molecules of which are large—say sulphate of magnesia—and the water on the other side the same percentage weight of a salt of smaller molecule—say chloride of sodium—water containing the salt of smaller molecule will pass into the water containing the salt of heavier molecule with a certain force. If, to start with, the two solutions are at the same level, the level of the solution containing the less diffusible salt, sulphate of magnesia, will at the commencement of the experiment rise. It is therefore said to exert a greater osmotic pressure than the more diffusible salt—chloride of sodium. Equilibrium will not be established until the fluid on one side of the membrane contains the same number of molecules per unit volume as the fluid on the other side. If the molecules of magnesic sulphate are pictured as oranges, and the molecules of sodic chloride as nuts, it will be understood that equilibrium is not established until the oranges and nuts to the pint on one side equal in number the oranges and nuts to the pint on the other. When these principles are applied to the passage of water containing products of digestion through the wall of the alimentary canal, it is evident that, if we understand all the condi- tions, the process cannot be explained as merely an exhibition of osmosis. Take the simplest illustration. When blood-serum is placed in the intestine it is absorbed. If it were in a dialyser, there would be equilibrium between the 2 is not a te ee ‘The cells which line the > take up substances far less easily diffusible than the hate of magnesia which they refuse. Nevertheless, speak- « Fy ng generally, iti is the less diffusible salts which act as aperients, a a of Gas contents” of the alimentary canal is a buatetied by castor-oil. ./ _ The peristalsis of the intestines is stimulated by certain drugs, ~~. _ such as jalap or the burnt products of tobacco. Another class 42 of drugs, of which aloes is an example, increases the secretion 4 of the intestines, small or large. Certain purgatives, such as ~ _ ¢alomel, podophyllin, etc., used to be regarded as cholagogues. / , ___ It was supposed that they increased the flow of bile. Thisis ~ aneerror. Their action is complicated, but it affects chiefly _ the peristalsis of the intestine. The poor misunderstood liver still suffers from the libels of primitive medical science. It is _ the most innocent of organs, in no way responsible for derange- ments of digestion. It carries out its functions without haste / | and without delay. With the possible exception of salicylate of soda, no drug is known which can stimulate it to a more rapid output of bile. ~ Absorption.—All the cells which line the alimentary canal are capable of absorbing food, if it is presented to them in a suitable form. In a suitable form means, speaking generally, “in a diffusible condition, although it must not be supposed that the epithelial cells are incapable, under certain circumstances, of taking up non-diffusible substances, just as a unicellular organism—an amoeba—can take in food. If soluble proteins, such as white of egg or acid-albumin, are injected into the large i ; ' serum inside the intestine and the lymph on the outside. There would be no osmosis. Or, again, supposing water containing 2% of common salt is placed in the intestine, we find that both salt and water pass through into the lymph. In a dialyser water would pass from the lymph (which contains salts equal to about 0°9 % of sodic chloride) through the membrane into the stronger solution. A salt-solution needs to be very concentrated to cause water to take the reverse course through the intestinal wall, and so to act as a purgative. When we study absorption from the alimentary canal, we find that its if it wants a salt or any other substance, sets the laws of osmosis a If the salt is not-wanted; the ordinary phenomena Of osmosis are exhibited. Sulphate of magnesia (Epsom salt) would be deleterious if absorbed. The intestinal wall behaves towards it like a dead membrane. The salt retains the water in which it is dissolved: possibly water passes out of the lymph into the solution of the salt. The contents of the intestines are in consequence unduly liquid. The salt acts as a purge. 9 ae ey ot) 4 ve? * ao THE BODY AT WORK | intestine, a very considerable proportion of ‘he subones so. injected is absorbed. It is possible, indeed, to supply in this = a way the whole of the nitrogenous food needed by the system, none entering by the mouth. If milk is injected, a certain amount of the fat also is retained. It can be shown that such absorption takes place when no digestion of the ‘food occurs in the colon. The food is taken up by the epithelial cells in the form in which it is in’ injected. The organs specially devoted to a absorption are the villi, which project into the contents of the small intestine. Haok | is a conical process about 0-5 millimetre long. The _ villi __ are longest_in the upper half of the small intestine. Below this level they decrease in number and size. A villus is completely covered with epithelial cells of short, columnar form. The free border of each cell is slightly hardened, forming a disc or cap which appears striated in optical section—an indication, as some think, that it is traversed by pores. Others hold that the appearance of striation is due to minute cilia-like projections which beset the free border of each cell. In worms and other invertebrates the cells carry motile projections of not inconsiderable size, which no doubt free their surfaces from the unassimilable matter which tends to accumulate upon them. Possibly they help to fix particles which are suitable for absorption. In mammals the presence _ of cilia has not been demonstrated. The extreme minuteness of the striz seems to point to their being merely indications that the border is permeable to fluids, including droplets of fat. The so-called basement membrane upon which the epithelial cells rest must not be regarded as a membrane in the physical sense. Rather is it a basket-work which supports the cells, without in any degree limiting their power of disgorging into the lymph-spaces of the villi the substances which they have absorbed. Within the villus, connective tissue forms a sponge- work, the spaces of which are filled with lymph, in which a con- siderable number of leucocytes | roam, on the look-out, no doubt, for any germs which may make their way between the epi- thelial cells. In the centre of the villus is a lymphatic radicle—z.e., a fusiform cul-de-sac—which is the dilated end of a lymph-vessel. It, like all other lymph-vessels, is walled by flattened endothelial scales. It communicates with the 131 Z y ieee The nenartinar poss ¢ vessels ae? in the MeNENtery 3 converge to the receptaculum chyli, the bulbous commence- [ oa ye... ’ “4 7 ment of the thoracic duct, which lies at the back of the abdomen in front of the bodies of the vertebre. The thoracic duct runs up the front of the vertebral column, through the thorax, and then hooks over to pour the fluid which it conveys into the great veins shortly before they join the heart. After a meal containing fat the fluid in the lymphatic vessels of the mesen- tery, the lacteals, has, as already stated (p. 43), the appearance of milk. The fat absorbed by the epithelium covering a villus is passed on into its lymph-space. From this into the central lacteal receptacle, thence to the submucous and peri- intestinal plexuses, the lacteal vessels of the mesentery, the thoracic duct. Absorbed fat does not pass_ through the. liver, but is carried into the heart ; thence through the lungs, and back to the heart, which pumps it to all parts of the body. In addition to the lacteal_radicle; the villus contains long capillary bloodvessels, and the arteriole and venule in which they commence and end. These traverse the lymph-spaces of the connective tissue, which contains, not only the fat which the epithelial cells have passed into it, but the other products of digestion also. None of the fat traverses the walls of the bloodvessels ; but the other products diffuse from the lymph, through the walls of the vessels, into the blood. Many nerve- fibres are found in the core of the villus on their way to epi- thelial cells, or to one or two plain muscle-fibres which are disposed in the direction of its long axis. For each villus is a “as pump. By the contraction of the muscle- fibres iba Sra noous vessels. Two problems have to be considered : First, in what form and by what mechanism are the several kinds of food ab- sorbed? Secondly, what becomes of them after they have been absorbed ? Clearly, the epithelial cell is the absorbing mechanism. It is not a membrane governed by the laws which regulate diffu- sion of fluids through membranes, but a living cell. There is hardly any limit to. its power of selecting { the food which it a 9—2 Cael ee mc eee 8 eigen AP ve SO eee ~ le ee eee) en ory eS m~ Pe tn ge ee Oe Se VR re oS ae ' ~ ; ; ee hee OTD bere its “ws = ie ’ poste, ne yh ay Oat ae me i yt ee a Saeki 132 THE BODY AT WORK ingests. It could, and very possibly it does, ingest albumin and bs | fats as such. Still, the elaborate provision which is made for __ converting albumin into diffusible peptone, and cane-sugar and maltose into easily diffusible dextrose, suggests that substances which will pass through membranes are more readily absorbed than substances which will not. We are justified in looking upon absorption as a physical problem up to a certain point. But we must not dwell too much on the physical aspects of the problem. If the absorption of food were merely a process of diffusion, an enormous quantity of water would be required. _ to carry the diffusible products of digestion into the villi. The passage of the foods is aided by the selective activity of the epithelial cells. Peptonization greatly facilitates the work of — the epithelial cells, but it is not a condition éssential to ab- sorption, so far as soluble proteins are concerned. It i is, how-_ ever, essential that the proteins should be presented to the epi- thelial cells in a soluble form. They could do nothing with the solid fibres of meat, however much they might have been dis- integrated by mastication and by the action of hydrochloric acid. It is only after digestion by pepsin and by trypsin that all the proteins of food are brought into solution. Digestion is needed to reduce them to a condition in which the epithelial cells can take them up. Much thought has been devoted to the question of the form in which fat is absorbed. Fat in the chemical sense—a pure fat, that is to say—is a compound of a fatty acid and glycerin. Suet, lard, butter, vegetable oils, etc., are mixtures of several fats. All consist of glycerin united with fatty acids. The acids are stearic acid, palmitic acid, oleic acid, and others of less importance. Fats are insoluble in water; so also are the fatty acids. A fatty acid combined with an alkali (in place of glycerin) is a soap. Soaps are soluble in water. If milk is examined under the microscope, it is found to contain droplets of fat, varying in size, but all minute. The larger droplets tend to rise to the surface as cream, but the smaller droplets do not run together. If milk from which the cream has been skimmed is sterilized, it retains its normal appearance for an indefinite time. Its fat remains in droplets. In technical language, milk is an emulsion. Theoretically oil and water would make an emulsion, if the droplets of oil were DIGESTION 133 rendered sufficiently minute. Sucha condition has been almost obtained by agitating oil and water with powdered glass. But the more viscous the medium through which oil globules are distributed, the greater is the resistance to their fusion. If oil which has become rancid—in which a certain quantity of fatty acid has been liberated from the glycerin with which, in a neutral fat, it is combined—is shaken with water containing carbonate of soda, an emulsion is easily formed. The carbonate of soda and the fatty acids form soaps. about twenty minutes after a meal. After ] bef ore fresh food. antl it. cp may be that the last meal | was too large or the interval too short. If the mucous membrane \ ‘ol is in an unhealthy condition, its own secretions afford material 4 on which bacteria thrive. Nothing short of washing it out with a stomach-pump will clean it up. The presence, at the time of feeding, of food left over from the previous meal is _ likely to perpetuate the unsatisfactory state of affairs. All _ the glands of the alimentary tract exhibit a tendency to periodicity. Their efficiency is greatest when activity follows. _ aperiod of rest. If the stomach is not able to expel its contents, it has not the opportunity of preparing for fresh duties. Fat_ undergoes a certain amount of rancid fermentation_ in the | stomach: Proteins are not attacked by bacteria in the stomach unless the condition of the organ is very unsatisfactory. The odour of the products of their decomposition is then recog- nizable in the breath. Bacteric fermentations in the small intestine are unimpor- \ tant under normal coriditions, with the exception of the | fermentation of cellulose. Cellulose has the same empirical | formula as starch. It is completely insoluble, and is not affected by any of the digestive juices. The greater part of the cellulose consumed by herbivora is, however, broken up by bacteria into acetic and butyric acids, carbonic acid, and marsh-gas. In Man also a small quantity is similarly. de- stroyed. In the large intestine the bacteric fermentations are not unlike those which occur in the stomach, with, in addition, the destruction of proteins, or of products of proteid digestion. The greater the quantity of undigested food which reaches the large intestine, the greater is the development of bacteria. When the stomach is dilated, the ascending colon, and-espe- cially | its cecum, is usually dilated also. Bacteric fermenta- tion in the large intestine, with resulting flatulence, is evidence of imperfect digestion, due either to an excess of food or to weakness of the alimentary organs, or, as is more commonly PAR: oS! ye ae = “ —" , 138 _ the case, to the combination of these two factors. The relation 3 % of fermentation to alimentation can be shown by counting the microbes in a specimen of the contents of the large intes- tine. In a particular case it fell from 65,000 per milligramme upon a mixed diet to 2,000 per milligramme upon a diet of milk. In the world at large bacteria perform many offices of the utmost usefulness to other living things. They fix nitrogen in — the soil, sweeten polluted rivers, reduce animal and. vegetable matter to a condition in which it is available as plant-food. _ Their presence within the alimentary canal is inevitable ; but it is somewhat doubtful whether, with the exception of the fermentation of cellulose, they do the economy any service with which it could not dispense. As parasites of the alimen- tary canal, some kinds are less desirable than others. Recently a method of limiting their variety has been introduced and advocated with much enthusiasm, as favourable to the hygiene of the digestive tract. In countries in which the cows are driven, in summer, to mountain pastures, the peasants of the plains live during their absence largely upon milk brought down at intervals, and allowed to turn sour. Sour milk, in Bulgaria, develops a bacterium of extraordinary vigour. It can live in a medium containing as much as 10 per cent. of lactic acid, a concentration fatal to other forms of Bacterium acidi lactict. It is easily cultivated, and when ingested continues to multiply in the alimentary canal. So peculiarly lusty is this bacterium that it makes life impossible for other germs. As it dies out after two or three months, it seems unlikely that a man who swallows the Bulgarian milk-germ runs a risk of inviting a repetition of the tragedy which followed the acclimatization of the mongoose in Jamaica. Its supremacy has been attri- buted to its capacity of developing a concentration of lactic acid too high for the well-being of other bacteria ; but it is improbable that it has the opportunity of doing this in the ali- mentary canal of a person living on a mixed diet. The extinc- tion of other bacteria (if they are extinguished) is more likely to be due to an antagonism of a more subtle kind, at present inexplicable, but not without parallel. The purifying influence of the water of the Ganges has for ages been an article of faith. Pilgrims from fever-stricken districts bathe in it, foul it, drink it, with the corpses of their fellows floating down the stream. ~ ee Sie Ao oe a ee Fe IN ee Ve OO) . «, 4 cated pl a oe oe eS > ee Sy * 5 ?- ’ . = e fer ir 4 ¥?- > = ao) DIGESTION 139° z Recently it has been shown that this belief is not without foundation. The water of the Ganges at Benares contains bacteria which are as tigers among lesser vermin. The germs of cholera and typhoid fever disappear from cultures into which these overbearing microbes are introduced. | Conditions Requisite for Normal Digestion.—When M. Chev- reul, Professor of Chemistry at the Jardins des Plantes of Paris, attained his hundredth year, an interviewer very natur- ally inquired of him, “ Have you always had a good diges- tion 2?” To this the still vigorous Professor answered: “I really cannot say, for I have never noticed.” So long as it is well used, the stomach is an unobtrusive organ. It is tyrannical when it deems itself the victim of inconsiderate treatment. A study of its physiology serves to show that it will work contentedly only upon certain clearly defined terms, of which the following are perhaps the most important: The ) stomach exacts due warning that its services are wanted. The nerves of smell and taste must announce the approach of food and guarantee its quality. ‘“‘ What may I eat ?”’ asked a large-framed, strenuous, eager, over-worked barrister of a great physician. ‘‘ Eat, sir? You may eat whatever you like. But be quite sure that you do like it.”’ Wise advice. The human race would not have developed its strong preferences for certain kinds of food if all foods were equally suitable to satisfy its needs. Taste is not a matter of fashion. It is the expression of the experience of mankind. Fanciful as civilization has made us, and easily as appetite is perverted, if we are sure that we really like, and want, a food, we may trust that our liking will guide us as safely as it guides a buffalo oradeer. ‘“‘ Eat what you like.” Hating with liking carries with it the idea of obtaining the maximum of satisfaction from the exercise of this necessary function. Most things which are reckoned unwholesome are full in flavour or rich in consistency. They satisfy the palate when spread out very thin. It is poor economy to help oneself to caviare with a table-spoon. In the second place, the stomach must be assured that the teeth are doing their proper share of work. Among the many half- truths which every year are exalted to the level of a revela- tion or a rule of conduct is the doctrine of the “ chewers ”— persons who take no meals, but industriously and almost “at a sitting.” In from two to three hours the last of aha food should have passed through the pylorus, allowing the stomach to rest before it is called into activity again. As proteins are practically the only foods which are digested in the stomach, the work required of this organ depends upon the quantity of proteins present amongst the constituents of a meal. Meat is the food richest in proteins, although bread, vegetables, milk, cheese also yield them. Some people can digest three meat meals every day ; but others, probably the majority, find that it is unwise to take any considerable quantity of meat more than once in twenty-four hours. It is only when the cells of the gastric glands have accumulated a store of pepsinogen-granules that proteid digestion is vigorously carried on. Fourthly, the food must be in a form in which it does not irritate the stomach, provoking an outflow of acid out of proportion to the pepsin which accompanies it. Ex- perience alone can teach the foods which are to be avoided on this account. But speaking generally, it may be said that the stomach resents the presence of substances which cannot be amalgamated into chyme. Its task is the reduction of the mixture of foods which compose a meal to the consistence of a smooth cream. Hot buttered toast or pie-crust are made of wholesome constituents enough, but, fat being melted into the starch, the fragments are impermeable to the gastric juice. They act mechanically as irritants of the mucous membrane. Again, it may be said that “ pure” foods are apt to provoke acidity. Nothing could be more wholesome than eggs or pounded meat or custard pudding; but taken by themselves these articles of diet over-stimulate the mucous membrane. They need to be diluted with starch-foods, or even with cellulose. _ And this calls attention to the dietetic value of vegetables. Vegetables, which consist chiefly of innutritious cellulose, _ distribute the digestible constituents of a meal and increase _ its bulk, greatly favouring its progress through the alimentary _ canal. Especially in herbivora is it important that the bulk - and looseness of the food should be well maintained. Rabbits thrive on sugar, starch, and albumin, mixed with such an DIGESTION 141 absolutely indigestible substance as horn-shavings. If the inert substance be omitted, they die of intestinal inflammation, although fed on the same mixture of pure foods. Other rules which govern digestion might be mentioned; and it is needless to point out that, when the mechanism is deranged, steps adapted to the particular malady must be taken to bring it back to a | normal condition. There is, however, one precaution upon which, ina certain number of cases, it is impossible to lay too much stress. The digestion of proteins is seldom carried out satis- factorily when much sugar, and especially much cane-sugar, has been eaten at the same meal. Excessive lactic fermenta- tion prevents the proper peptonization of meat. The chemistry of digestion is not sufficiently well understood to enable the physiologist to say what is amiss ; but probably by-products of peptic digestion are produced. To many people this is of little consequence ; but to those who exhibit a gouty tendency it is, unfortunately, a most serious matter. Civilized races are particularly subject to the uric acid diathesis. In the course of nitrogenous metabolism uric acid is formed in place of fully oxidized and easily soluble neutral urea. Although the chemical sequence has not been discovered as yet, there is no question but that imperfect gastric digestion means the forma-_ tion of uric acid, with all its lugubrious results : malaise, neck- ache, emotional depression. Birds and reptiles form uric acid as the end-product of nitrogenous metabolism, not urea. So also do city-fathers, butchers, and others whose diet consists too largely of meat. Many nervous, ill-nourished men and women tend to do the same, however abstemious their meals. It is useless to tell such persons to reduce the amount of proteins in their diet. Their attempts at increasing the starch, sugar, and fat at the expense of nitrogenous foods lead to dyspepsia, which makes matters worse. They often find, however, that if they are careful to restrict to the narrowest limits the amount of carbohydrates (especially sugar) which they take in conjunction with meat, fish, eggs, or other proteid foods, the formation of uric acid ceases. Sugar, bread, fruit, and other carbohydrates, may be taken in abundance, and with great advantage, at breakfast and lunch, without proteid food, if dinner consists of broth, fish, meat, cheese, vegetables, with a minimum of bread. The History of the Foods after ‘Absorptios. Alt for Pe with the exception of inorganic salts and salts of various — vegetable acids, fall into three classes: (1) Proteins—sub- — stances of complex chemical constitution, containing nitro- gen; (2) carbohydrates—so called because hydrogen and oxygen, in the proportions in which they enter into the forma- tion of water, are united with carbon ; (3) fats. Proteins of various kinds are consumed as food. The peptones produced from them by digestion also vary. Yet very little is known as to the differences in physiological value which distinguish the various kinds of protein when absorbed into the fluids of the body (cf. p. 134). All carbohydrates after digestion and absorp- tion appear as dextrose. The various fats preserve their in- dividuality until they are taken up by the tissues. When fixed in the tissues, they assume, except under somewhat abnormal conditions, the composition characteristic of the fat of the animal which has eatenthem. Ifa dog which has been severely starved is fed upon mutton-fat, it puts on in the first instance fat which resembles that of a sheep rather than the normal fat of a dog. As soon, however, as it is well nourished (which would never occur unless some protein and carbohydrate were added to the mutton-fat), its fat assumes the usual form. For practical purposes we are obliged to speak of the three classes of food—proteid, carbohydrate, and fatty—as if there were but one member in each class. And we have abundant evidence that such a simple classification is fully justified. The body has so large a power of altering chemically the nature of the food which it absorbs that it makes little difference in the further history of the food whether the protein supplied to it be an albumin or a globulin; the fat, stearin, palmitin, or olein ; the carbohydrate, starch or sugar. In earlier days it was customary to regard the body as the receiver of a. variety of foods which it could break down into simpler substances by oxidation, but could not reconstruct. Plants were regarded as the manufacturers of organic com- pounds, animals as the destroyers of the complex substances made by plants. The union of molecules, synthesis, was looked upon as the function of the vegetable kingdom. Animals built into their tissues the products elaborated by plants ; some of these products they shook to pieces for the a a “a re 0 ieee pire energy sph others sslowly disintegrated — S result. of tissue “‘ wear and tear.” Gradually it was realized that many chemical changes occur ii ate bods oh cannot be viewed as merely ‘exhibitions — of its analytical - capacity. The tissues were recognized, as laboratories ii _ which reactions occur which consist in something more than the _ splitting of complex into simpler molecules. The instances earliest understood were connected with the history of carbo- _ hydrates and fats. In the disease diabetes an enormous _ quantity of sugar is excreted, amounting in extreme cases to between 1 and 2 pounds per diem. When carbohydrates are present in the food, the amount of sugar excreted in diabetes is greater than it is when they are withheld; on an almost exclusively proteid diet the amount of sugar excreted far exceeds the amount of carbohydrates in the food. Another illustration of the power of making sugar possessed by the animal economy is afforded by a dog fed upon lean meat, and nothing else. Sugar is found in its blood, and a store of carbo- hydrate (glycogen) in its liver. The formation of fat is an instance of constructive metabolism. There is abundant evidence that the quantity of fat produced may greatly exceed the quantity contained in the food. Animals are fattened for the market on a diet which contains less fat than that which accumulates in their bodies. When nursing her young, an animal may secrete in her milk much more fat than she obtains as such in food. It was a great mistake to suppose that the body is dependent upon its tradesmen for fat and _ Sugar. It can make either of these substances out of a mixed diet in which it is relatively deficient. It must, however, be a mixed diet. An animal cannot live exclusively on fat or exclusively on carbohydrate. It is impossible, therefore, for us to determine whether, if given the one alone, it can turn it into the other. Chemists were very unwilling to credit the body with the power of performing even the simpler of these trans- formations—the conversion of carbohydrate into fat. Proteins are essential constituents of a fattening diet. Their immensely complex molecule has always afforded a tempting field for arithmetical ingenuity. It is easy to remove from it the atoms needed for the composition of fat, and yet to leave such groups of atoms as might reasonably be supposed to con- va ss ee stitute its ‘‘ nitrogenous moicty The Wide that thee : metabolic capacity of the body is limited to analytical pro- = cesses justified the supposition that, when more fat is laid on ; than the food contains, the balance comes from proteid sub- _ stances, which split into nitrogenous and fatty moieties. It has been shown, however, that an animal during fattening may put on more fat than is contained as such in the food, or obtainable from its diet, even though all the atoms of carbon and hydrogen in its proteid food were devoted to its forma- tion. The balance must come from carbohydrates. Perhaps a still more striking illustration of constructive capacity is the power of making glycerin. If a dog receive fatty acids in its diet, it accumulates normal fats. The glycerin which, united with fatty acids, constitutes the fat, was not contained in its food. Starch and sugar are sources of fat. As yet there is no evidence that fat can be converted into sugar. The chemistry of ‘he nitrogen-containing compounds appears to present more difficult problems. Plants build up proteins. Is the animal’s relation to these substances limited to their disintegration ? Do proteins inevitably descend from step to step until they reach urea? There are reasons for think- ing that, even when dealing with nitrogenous substances, the metabolic power of the body is not exclusively ana- lytical. The liver can make urea from ammonia-salts, such as lactate, or even carbonate, of ammonia—substances more stable, and therefore in the chemical sense simpler, than urea. This is an indication, though a faint one, that the body has a constructive capacity, a power of producing more complex from simpler substances, even in the case of nitrogenous compounds. Beef-tea, mutton broth, meat-extracts have long been regarded as foods of value when the power of assimilation is low. Chemists point out that the nitrogenous substances which these decoctions contain are so near the bottom of the ladder that the energy set free by their further oxidation to urea is scarcely worth consideration. 'They admit that their ready availability renders them useful as restoratives, but they deny them the status of foods, on the assumption that their further progress must be downward. As was stated when the conversion of peptones into leucin and tyrosin was described, evidence is be- ginning to accumulate which shows that within certain limits, at De ce | ipossible to define, the system can reconstruct its ‘from. amides and other simple products of their - + ~ ~ 4 a Tah x 5 i we se = * LeoTradation. _ The animal economy receives, and after due digestive pre- _ paration absorbs, three classes of food—nitrogenous, fatty, and _ carbohydrate. If either of the two latter kinds be deficient _ in the diet, the body can to a certain extent produce it from the other two. What is the special value of each kind of food ? - What use is made of it? Before attempting to answer these questions, we must endeavour to trace the further history of the foods after they have traversed the wall of the alimentary canal. After leaving the stomach and intestines, the foods follow two different routes. Proteins and carbohydrates are carried by the portal vein to the liver. Fats are carried by the thoracic duct to the general circulation. An excess of fat is found in the blood in all parts of the body after a meal rich in fat. The eventual destination and fate of fatty foods is unknown. Under certain circumstances they are added to the fatty deposits in connective tissue ; but if no additional fat is being laid down, they go to other tissues, in which they are oxi- dized into carbonic acid and water. When the amount absorbed is excessive, a certain quantity of fat may be stored in the liver. In the cells of this organ it is housed for a time, in order that it may be distributed to the tissues after they have used up the supplies which first reach them through the general blood-stream. Proteins are completely lost to sight after they are ab- sorbed into the blood. They take part, of course, in the formation of growing tissue, blood-corpuscles, skin, hair, nails. It is also common to speak of them as making good the wear and tear- of active tissues, although it is very doubtful whether we can legitimately speak of the wear and tear of tissues. The protoplasm which does the work of the body is not worn out in the same way as the materials of which a machine is made. There is no friction to rub it down. Proteins, like other foods, are used up as sources of muscular energy and heat. Eventually they are reduced to urea, carbonic acid, and water. Chemists naturally seek for substances inter- Mediate in constitution between proteins and urea. They 10 ee oe » _ e we THE BODY AT WORK oO ee eee ae ee ee bib fv) Kee, isp Sidi assume that the degradation of proteins will occur in regule rs steps; complex, partially oxidized, nitrogenous compounds 5 being formed first—in the muscles, for example—to be further oxidized in the glands. The existence in all organs of nitro- — genous “ extractives,’ which can be separated out when the organ is subjected to chemical analysis, seems to justify the search for stages ; but hitherto this search has been singularly unsuccessful. Urea is the final product. It is not found in muscle, nor, indeed, in any tissue other than the liver, which, as already said, has the power of making it, even from salts of ammonia. It is therefore clear that if proteins are destroyed in muscle and other tissues, and if all urea is made by the liver, the antecedents of urea must be carried from the muscles to this organ. The substance which is most characteristic of — muscular metabolism is lactic acid. It is not impossible that all the nitrogenous portion of the complex proteid molecule is reduced to ammonia (NH), which may be regarded as the simplest of all nitrogenous compounds, and that this, com- bined with lactic acid (C,;H,O;) as lactate of ammonia (NH,C,H,O,), is carried by the general circulation to the liver, where it is converted into urea. A considerable amount of lactate of ammonia may be injected into a vein without any of it overflowing through the kidneys. It is all reduced to the condition of urea, water, and carbonic acid. If the liver is so diseased as to be functionless, or if by operative measures it is thrown out of action, salts of ammonia are excreted by the kidneys instead of urea. In birds and reptiles uric acid takes the place of urea. Their livers yield uric acid on analysis. If lactate of ammonia be injected into their blood, it is con- verted into uric acid, so long as the liver is intact. We know nothing of the forms assumed by the proteins absorbed into the blood, of the organs in which they are stored, or of the higher terms of the series of substances through which they pass before they are finally excreted as urea, water, and carbonic acid. No nitrogenous compounds are found in lymph or blood which can be pointed out with confi- dence as the products of tissue wear and tear. When consider- ing the sources of muscular energy, we shall have something more to sayregarding the part that proteins playin the economy. If there is great difficulty in following fats and proteins - DIGESTION 147 after their absorption, it is quite otherwise when we come to deal with sugar. Carbohydrates are the great sources of energy. Muscular work may be generated by the oxidation of either of the three classes of foods, but undoubtedly the carbohydrate glycogen is its most constant source. Pro- _ vision is therefore made for the storing of glycogen in the liver, and the distribution to the muscles of a regular supply. After a meal the portal blood, on its way from the intestines tothe liver, contains a higher percentage of sugar than the blood in the hepatic vein or in any other vessel. If sections of liver be examined after feeding, and compared with those obtained after a period of starvation, it is found that the cells of the well-fed liver contain glancing masses of a sub- stance which takes a port-wine colour with iodine. This is glycogen, or animal starch. It has the same empirical formula as starch (Cj;H,,0;),. In the dry state it is a greyish powder, which, unlike starch, forms an opalescent solution in cold water. Like starch, it is non-diffusible. In the animal king- dom it stands to sugar in the same relation as starch to sugar in plants. If a sheep be killed while it is feeding in the paddock, and its liver removed and weighed, it will be found that it is from one-third to one-half heavier than the liver of a sheep of the same weight obtained from a butcher ; for butchers have the stupid practice of starving animals before they killthem. It was long ago discovered that it is unneces- sary to feed an animal for a day or two before it is killed, and this option has been elevated into a prohibition. A tradition has grown up that it is undesirable to give food for some time before killing. Not only will the liver of a sheep killed during active digestion be found to be heavier than that of a starved sheep, but it will also prove more succulent ; for it is loaded with sugar (into which glycogen is rapidly con- verted after death), as well as with proteins and fats, which are withdrawn from it when the animal fasts. It appears that the liver cannot secure the whole of the sugar which is absorbed after a full meal. Some of it passes into the general circulation, and is stored in the muscles; but the liver always maintains a considerable reserve. Even after prolonged deprivation of food, it holds on to a certain quantity, especially in carnivora. Smoeen 3 is found in the liver of a dog after a : 10—2 “148 THE BODY AT WORK ae The muscles lose during activity q e glycogen which -they.c = It has already been pointed out that the ea is not onkinel 4 dependent upon external agencies for the production of the — sugar which it needs. When the supply is inadequate, it manu-_ : factures glycogen for itself out of the other constituents of the diet. It can, indeed, make it at the expense of its own proteins. If a dog which has been caused to do muscular work, without a sufficiency of carbohydrate food, until (as judged from a control experiment) all glycogen has disappeared from its liver, be placed under the influence of a narcotic drug, which arrests the activity of its muscles, glycogen reappears. Dietetics.— Even those who are most ignorant of the science of physiology flatter themselves that they have one piece of information : “‘ The whole of the body is renewed once in every seven years.” I cannot trace the origin of this sapient apo- thegm, which for generations has passed current. If seven weeks or seventy years were the period allowed for the renewal of the tissues, the statement would be equally near the truth. Judging from the rate at which they are destroyed, it is unlikely that blood-corpuscles live for more than five or six weeks. Hairs are shed about two years after they first appear above the surface. On attaining this age a hair drops off and a new one takes its place. The superficial cells of the skin are shed in great numbers every day, and their place taken by younger cells which come up from the deeper layers. The cells of many glands would seem to have a comparatively short term of life. On the other hand, some tissue-elements are far more perma- nent. By the time a child is a year old all its nerve-cells are in _ position. They last as long as the individual lives. When | the statement with regard to the renewal of the tissues is understood as meaning, not that the cells are destroyed and replaced by new ones, but that within a period of seven years all the molecules which enter into their protoplasm are ex- truded from the body and replaced by molecules received as food, the assertion verges on the transcendental. It is unlikely © that we shall ever obtain data against which it can be checked. The essential part of every living cell is its spongework of protoplasm. ‘‘Bioplasm ”’ is perhaps a better term to use when we are speaking of protoplasm as a structure, since it does not Vegex: any rejbitios with regard to its chemical constitution. W: ithir the meshes of the bioplasm are nutrient materials, s yet unused, and worked-up products in various stages. It as always been taken for granted that when treating of nutri- ti on, we have to consider the repair of the bioplasm, as well as the provision of raw material which it can convert into the specific products of the cell. Suppose that the cell belongs _ to the class of supporting tissues ; let it be a cell of cartilage, for example. The bioplasm manufactures a collagenous sub- stance which remains in and around its meshwork. If it be an epidermal cell, it forms horny substance. If a secreting cell, it accumulates secernable products. If a muscle-cell, it _ develops a large quantity of material, which by a change in _ form produces movement. In this last case we suppose that the energy set free as muscular force is due to oxidation. More stable bodies take the place of a less stable substance. After contraction the relatively complex contractile material is renewed from the foods stored in the muscle-cell ; or if it be not, in the ordinary sense of the word, destroyed, if it has merely parted with certain oxidizable constituents, it obtains a fresh supply of such constituents from the foods which the muscle-cell contains. Even in the case of cartilage or epidermis, we imagine that, since the matrix is “ alive,” it is always under- going molecular change, and consequently always requiring food. The fact that every tissue, however inert, dies when, owing to the blocking of the bloodvessels which irrigate the part, its supply of nutriment is cut off, justifies this belief that all living tissue is undergoing change. When we make up a balance-sheet of the body as a whole, _ placing to the debit side the food which it receives, and to its credit side the work done in external movement and in the production of heat, we again find reason for believing that every part of every cell is constantly undergoing change. _ The balance-sheet of the body can be drawn out in either of two ways. We can estimate the quantities of nitrogen, carbon, hydrogen, and oxygen supplied to it in the several foods, and compare them with the amounts of each of these four elements given off in urea, carbonic acid, and water, making, of course, a note of the body’s balance in hand at 150 THE BODY AT WORK — the beginning and at the end of the period of observation. Or, we may estimate the amount of potential energy contained in the food, and ascertain the use to which this energy is put in doing external work, in maintaining the temperature of the body, and in warming the breath and other excreta. If we are making up the balance-sheet of a fully-grown man, we may take for granted that he is not making fresh tissue. During the period throughout which he is under observation, care is taken to avoid altering the conditions of his life in such a manner as to lead him to develop additional muscle. If he gains in weight while under observation, he is putting on fat. If he loses in weight, he is sacrificing fat. The whole of the nitrogen taken in leaves the body in urea, unless, as we have said, growth of tissue is taking place. The body has not the same temptation to store nitrogen as it has to store carbon. Consequently, it is very sensitive to any deficiency of nitrogen in the diet. If food does not contain as much protein as is needed, the deficit is made up at the expense of the tissues. It does not necessarily follow that under these circumstances a man loses in weight. He may be putting on fat, although losing in strength owing to waste of muscle. For observations upon the income and expenditure of the body to be of any value, a condition of “ nitrogenous equilibrium ”’ must be established. The nitrogen taken in must equal in amount the nitrogen given out. Very exact determinations of income and expenditure may be made by placing an animal, or even a man, in a box through which airis drawn. A record is made of the volume of air drawn through the box. The percentages of water vapour and carbonic acid which the air contains are estimated before it enters and after it leaves. The solid food consumed and the urea excreted are also measured. If it is desired to measure the amount of heat given off, an animal may be placed in a calorimeter. Even when most passive, the subject under examination, whether an animal or a man, is expending energy in keeping the body warm, in movements of respiration, and in shifting position. If it is desired to ascertain the relation of oxidation to external work, it is easy to devise a form of resistance, such ; DIETETICS 151 as the turning of a wheel, or the lifting of a weight which can be measured. | | In testing diets, it suffices to make sure that nitrogenous equilibrium is maintained, and then to estimate the gain or loss in weight and the output of energy in external work. The Relative Value of Foods.—Dried proteins contain about 15 per cent. nitrogen, 54 per cent. carbon, 7 per cent. hydrogen, 22 per cent. oxygen, a little sulphur, and frequently some phosphorus. A large proportion of their carbon and hydrogen is available for combustion. Fats contain 75 per cent. of carbon, and a considerable quantity of hydrogen avail- able for combustion ; carbohydrates, 40 per cent. of carbon, with hydrogen and oxygen in the proportions in which they occur in water. If 1 gramme of protein is oxidized to the con- dition of urea, carbonic acid, and water, sufficient heat is liber- ated to raise the temperature of 4,100 grammes of water 1 degree centigrade. Its calorific value is therefore ex- pressed as 4,100 calories, the unit of measurement—a calorie— being the amount of heat needed to raise 1 gramme of water 1°. The calorific value of 1 gramme of fat is 9,300 calories ; of 1 gramme of starch, 4,100 calories. Thus, the energy poten- tial in protein and in starch is the same; that in fat more than twice as great as that in either of the other foods. A Normal Diet.—Nitrogenous equilibrium and body-weight can be maintained and work done on diets which vary widely in percentage composition. This is a question which we shall consider at greater length later on. In the meantime, for the sake of illustration, it is necessary to formulate a diet which is fairly representative of the selection of foods made by a man of average weight—say 70 kilogrammes (145 pounds)—who desires to do a moderate day’s work in comfort. It has been found to amount to about 100 grammes of protein, 100 grammes of fat, 240 grammes of carbohydrate, all measured dry and as pure foods. If the several elements of such a diet be multiplied by the figures which represent their calorific value, it will be found that the man is supplied with 2,324,000 calories. The illustration that we have chosen is the diet of a professional man who is not engaged in hard physical work. The pure foods would be found to the amounts stated in 17 ounces lean meat, 4 ounces butter, and 17 ounces bread. The day’s 152 THE BODY AT WORK diet would, of course, be much more varied than this, but itis simpler to express it in these terms. | et Such a diet would hardly answer the requirements of a man doing hard muscular work. Experience shows that he would expect to receive a more liberal supply of energy, and that to obtain it he would increase slightly his allowance of proteins, and very considerably increase the quantity of carbohydrates that he consumed. The diet of European workmen is remarkably constant in the relative amounts of its several constituents, no matter what their nationality or the exact form of their work may be: Proteins, about 135 grammes ; fats, 80 grammes ; carbohydrates, 500 to 700 grammes—giving a supply of energy © equal to 3,500 to 4,000 kilo-calories. Speaking generally, carbohydrates are the source of muscular force, and fats of heat. In warm climates men work on carbo- hydrates. The ’rickshaw men of Japan are said to eat only rice on working days, and to reserve fish for days of leisure. The Japanese, as is well known, consume extremely little fat. The Esquimaux and other inhabitants of high latitudes eat immense quantities of fat. Proteins constitute the luxurious element of a diet. Not only are they more attractive to most palates, and therefore preferred by persons whose dietary is not severely regulated by price, but the body prefers them. It works with greater alacrity when supplied with more protein than, in a strictly physiological sense, it needs. The supply of food must exceed the apparent demand. The most efficient of motors cannot convert more than 15 per cent. of the energy potential in its fuel into work. If aman endeavours to obtain a better result than this from his muscular system, if he tries to make his machine do more than 15 units of work for every 100 units of energy with which he supplies it, he does it at the expense of his own tissues. First he loses in weight, owing to the consumption of fat ; then the excess of nitrogen discharged over nitrogen consumed shows that he is burning up the proteins of his own tissues. It is needless to add that the weakness which results puts a stop to excessive work. Muscles, as we shall find when we consider the relation of their output of work to the energy supplied to them, can produce a much better result than the best of engines; but we are speaking of the body as a whole, which wastes energy in the _ a DIETETICS 153 a - movements of respiration, masticating food, shifting position, maintaining the body-temperature, etc. Health may be maintained and work done on diets which depart widely from the one which we have selected as a stan- dard. Darwin found the Guanchos of South America living exclusively on meat. Nansen and Johannsen, when seeking the North Pole, lived for months on meat and blubber. Millions of the inhabitants of India abstain from meat and meat-fat, their diet consisting of rice, buttermilk, and a little fruit. In the case of all persons with whom the price of food is an important consideration, carbohydrates are preferred to proteins and fats. Oatmeal is very much cheaper per unit of energy than meat. A man may be a meat-eater or a vegetarian, although he is probably unwise in overlooking the obvious teaching of his teeth and digestive organs, which are those of an omnivorous animal. His prehistoric human ancestors lived chiefly on the harvest of their spears and tomahawks. If we insist upon looking back still farther, we discern a cleavage of the race into the arboreal fruit-eaters, which still retain pre-human characters, and the more enterprising and energetic troglodyte hunters from whom the human race was evolved. A man may vary his diet within wide limits. In- numerable considerations lead certain individuals to desire to depart from the diet which we have termed “ normal ”—+7.e., typical of inhabitants of the temperate zone. One man rebels against the expense of living ; he would fain reduce the quan- tity and the cost of food. Another, having to traverse regions in which food is scarce, wishes to ascertain the lightest, and therefore the most portable, combination of its essential elements. A third—and he belongs to a much larger class— tormented with indigestion or harassed by gout, asks, ““ Why must I consume things which give the stomach trouble, or produce disagreeable and incapacitating after-effects ?”? Many circumstances prompt to experiments in diet. Much latitude is undoubtedly allowed. But there are limits within which alone health can be maintained and work done. It is of great interest to ascertain exactly how wide these limits are; and especially important is it to find out the lower limit, the minimum of food, and the minimum of each particular kind of food, which will enable the human machine to work. The problems —r tcl Shay ee A 154 THE BODY AT WORK involved are somewhat complicated. If it were possible to live on a single food, it would be as easy to ascertain the irre- ducible minimum as it is to find out with how much coal or with how much petrol an engine can be made to turn a wheel. But:to support the body several different kinds of food are in- dispensable. It is therefore necessary to determine, not only the minimum quantity of the combined foods, but also the minimum amount of each kind of food, and the effect upon the total of variations in the relative amount of each of its several factors. The problem is complicated, but certain limits are impassably defined. In the first place, with regard to the total amount, the work which the body does cannot under any circumstances be reduced below a certain level. The food consumed must provide a supply of energy equal, at the least, to the performance of the minimum of work. The body must receive each day food of due caloric value. Then with regard to the amount of each several constituent. Many considerations lead us to wish to increase one of them or to diminish another. Some food is cheap, and other food is dear. Economic reasons are in favour of the cheaper food. Even ethical considerations are not without weight. We have, perhaps, a prejudice against sacrificing life to supply the pot. We have doubts as to whether our system can properly digest, metabolize, and excrete meat. We need an unambiguous answer to the question, To what extent can nitrogen-foods be replaced by carbon-foods, and vice versa? A cell, as already said, consists of a framework of bioplasm bathed in cell-juice which contains nutrient substances and manufactured pro- ducts. The bioplasm is alive; the proteins, carbohydrates, and fats of the cell-juice are the materials with which it is nourished, and upon which it works. Some physiologists incline to the view that non-living substances must enter into the bioplasm before they undergo metabolism. They consider that the molecules of the non-living substance must at the time when they undergo a chemical change be physically and chemically a part of the living substance. Others take the opposite view: that the living substance does not undergo change, but brings about changes in the non-living substance which is in contact with it, enclosed within its meshes. This is a problem which is not likely to be solved, nor is its solution DIETETICS 155 of great importance in relation to the question which we are discussing. Whichever of the two views be justified, we have to distinguish between the bioplasm of the cell—the machine— and its raw materials and manufactured products. The question to which we want an answer is the following : Must the bioplasm undergo change ? There seems to be no reason in the nature of things why it should. It is not, as we have already pointed out, subject to wear and tear. A _ perfect machine would in the absence of friction, which rubs down its steel and brass, continue to turn out its products so long as it was supplied with raw materials and the energy needed to manufacture them. We could imagine the bioplasm as in- destructible, receiving energy from a portion of the foods, and expending this energy in the production of chemical change in the remainder. We could imagine that when once the tissues had attained their full growth they would require no more protein for their own nutrition ; they would be occupied in producing heat and motion from the non-nitrogenous foods. But observation shows clearly that this is not the case. The force which energizes the bioplasm, enabling it to evoke meta- bolism in non-living substance, is obtained at the cost of its own destruction. The bioplasm wastes unless constantly supplied with proteid food. . Under ordinary circumstances the amount of urea excreted varies directly as the quantity of nitrogen contained in the food. Since urea contains 45 per cent. of nitrogen, and protein 15 per cent., every gramme of urea excreted represents 3 grammes of dry protein consumed ; or, in terms of nitrogen, every gramme of nitrogen excreted represents 6-25 grammes of protein consumed. [If all food is withheld, the excretion of nitrogen falls, but it never reaches zero. Many observations have been made on fasting men. On the second day of fasting the nitrogen excreted falls to about 13 grammes, representing 80 grammes of protein used up. It is generally thought that by the second day all “ floating proteins ” are exhausted, and that therefore nitrogenous metabolism is reduced, as it were, to a business basis. So long as the supply of food is abundant, the body has a luxurious habit of using proteins in preference to non-nitrogenous food. But after a day’s starvation there is no longer any fancy metabolism, 156 THE BODY AT WORK no consumption of proteins as fuel when cheaper fats and sugar - would answer equally well. In the case of Succi, who fasted for thirty days, the nitrogen excreted fell to 6-7 grammes on the tenth day, to 4:3 grammes on the twentieth, and to 3-2 grammes on the last day. Clearly, we have to make a distinction, when all food is cut off, between the oxidation of the protein which, failing all other material, is withdrawn from the tissues for the purpose of supplying the force absolutely necessary to maintain respiration and such other movements as are inevitable, and to keep up the temperature of the body—force which under other circumstances might be supplied by non-nitrogenous food—and the oxidation to which bioplasm is inevitably subject, so long as it is alive. The oxidation of bioplasm under ordinary circumstances of course supplies force ; but it does not follow that this is sufficient to maintain the respiratory movements and the contraction of the heart. When a herbivorous animal is starved, it not infrequently excretes more urea at the commencement of the starvation period than it was excreting when well fed. Its activities did not come to a standstill when carbohydrate food was cut off. Fora time they were maintained at the expense of its own tissues. On the other hand, the results obtained from the observation of the man who went without food for thirty days show that Nature is able to economize force by reducing the metabolism of living substance below the normal. It might be supposed that the irreducible metabolism could be ascer- tained by giving a nitrogen-starved animal non-nitrogenous food, but it is found that this scarcely affects the tissue-waste. Becoming more active, the tissues, while saved from the neces- sity of supplying fuel for the production of heat and motion, suffer more waste. Again, it might be expected that if to an animal which had been starved for a few days, until its urea had fallen to the starvation limit, exactly sufficient protein were given to supply this amount, the tissues would be saved. It is found, on the contrary, that nearly twice as much urea is excreted as before. If the quantity of protein be steadily increased, equilibrium is at last established, but not until the amount of nitrogen in the protein given is two and a half times as great as the amount excreted during the starvation period. Additional food at once gives rise to additional _ DIETETICS ~ 157 waste. The tissues which during the period of scarcity had reduced their oxidation to a minimum become more active at the first hint of returning plenty. This last experiment illustrates a general law. An increase of proteid food within certain limits increases the metabolic activity of the tissues—provokes them to extravagance. It is possible, by adding protein to a mixed diet which sufficed for the maintenance of body-weight and nitrogenous equili- brium, to bring about a nitrogen deficit and to reduce the body-weight. Or, if the body is gaining in weight, owing to the accumulation of fat, the substitution of protein for carbo- hydrate (weight for weight, since their caloric values are the same) will lead to its reduction. It is difficult to avoid the use of fanciful language in accounting for these results. The animal economy is like an over-careful housekeeper, who, when meat is scarce, doles out porridge also with a thrifty hand. When meat is plentiful she is prodigal with every article of diet. Protein is the most costly of foods. Any indication that it is scarce leads to a shutting-down of activity. On the other hand, no other food is so readily absorbed (unless the digestive organs be protein-sick) ; none is so quickly incor- porated in the bioplasm ; none is so easy to decompose. When fed with protein the machinery hums. The insatiable appe- tite for beef and eggs which overtakes a man of sedentary habits after a long morning in a boat or on a bicycle does not indicate that his muscular tissue is suffering from wear and tear. It does not prove that he is setting free energy by oxidizing proteid food. It shows that he is asking certain tissues which are accustomed to a quiet life to exhibit pro- digious energy. They will not shake off their customary sloth unless he stimulates them with sumptuous fare. At the end of a week he finds that proteins are not the best fuel for steady work. If he consumes sufficient to supply all the energy needed by his muscles, he is hampered by a quantity of nitro- genous residues which have to be reduced to urea and elimi- nated by the kidneys. He goes back approximately to his old regimen, so far as proteins are concerned, and consumes more carbohydrates for the supply of the force which his increased muscular activity demands. It is possible to live on meat alone, but the quantity required 158 - THE BODY AT WORK is very great, involving the digestive organs, the liver, and the kidneys in an excessive amount of work. On the other hand, it is possible to reduce the consumption of proteins to a minimum by substituting for them fats and carbohydrates. But, again, after the proper balance is disturbed, the substitu- tion ceases to be a simple problem in arithmetic. The carbon- food has to be increased out of all proportion to the protein which it replaces. If a dog which is being fed on a diet natural toit—chiefly meat—is in a condition of nitrogenous equilibrium, carbohydrate may be substituted for some of the meat. But from the very beginning it is found that, if nitrogenous equili- brium is not to be disturbed (if the dog is not to be induced to consume its own tissues), a weight of carbohydrate must be given considerably greater than the weight of the protein withdrawn. The disproportion increases as the experiment proceeds, until perhaps 12 to 15 grammes of carbohydrate have to be substituted for every gramme of protein. The proteid food has now come down to 1-5 gramme per kilo- gramme of the animal’s weight. Owing to the increase of carbohydrate, the caloric value of the total food, nitrogenous and non-nitrogenous, is several times as great as the animal requires. The surplus is oxidized without any equivalent in work. At about this point the experiment is brought to an end, owing to the failure of the digestive organs to deal with so large a mass of food. The value of gelatin as an article of diet is of interest in this connection. Gelatin is not, strictly speaking, a protein, and it cannot be built up into the tissues. It does not prevent, nor even delay, starvation. Yet up to a certain point it can be used as a substitute for proteid food. In the observation just referred to, protein might be withdrawn at any stage, without disturbing nitrogenous equilibrium, by substituting” about 2 grammes of gelatin for every gramme of protein withdrawn. It spares protein, although it does not take its place. It is said that the minimum of protein necessary for the maintenance of nitrogenous equilibrium may be reduced to about one-half by the substitution of gelatin. This has been interpreted as indicating that when we have reduced the oxidation of nitrogenous substance to its smallest amount the nitrogen comes from two sources in about equal propor- >= - tions—(a) the bioplasm ; (6) the food-proteins in contact with it. It is inferred that gelatin, although it cannot be built up into bioplasm, may take the place of proteins present in the cell-juice. It appears to be impossible to starve the cell until it consists of a bioplasm framework bathed in nitrogen- free cell-juice. As the non-living proteins of cell-juice are removed, they are, if no nitrogenous food be given, renewed by the breaking down of bioplasm. When gelatin is absorbed, it takes its place in the cell-juice, and the breaking down of bioplasm is no longer necessary. When digestion is impaired, or vitality lowered, decoctions of meat which contain extrac- tives of low calorific value, useless, without synthesis (cf. p. 144), for the purposes of tissue-repair, may to a certain extent save tissue-waste. In the same way, gelatin, which is very rapidly digested in the stomach, may cover the consump- tion of proteins, although it cannot take their place. To sum up: The requisite daily income of energy must come from both nitrogenous and non-nitrogenous food. It is impossible to reduce the nitrogenous factor below a certain minimum. From this minimum upwards, until a certain level is reached, every additional unit of nitrogenous food enables the system to dispense with more than its equivalent of non- nitrogenous food. When the proper balance of foods is attained, there is no waste either of labour involved in digestion, or of labour involved in metabolism and excretion. The Liver.—The liver weighs from 3 to 34 pounds. It lies beneath the diaphragm, more on the right side than on the left. Its posterior border, which rests against the last three ribs (separated from them by the diaphragm), is about 3 inches thick. Its anterior border is thin, and keeps close along the line of the ribs. If the organ is neither unduly enlarged nor squeezed out of its place owing to the use of a tight corset, it does not project below the ribs, save where it crosses the space between the rib-cartilages below the end of the breast- bone. The liver is supplied with blood by the hepatic artery. This vessel is small for so large an organ. Although responsible for the nutrition of the liver, it does not bring it the materials which are stored in its cells. A much larger supply of blood is derived from the portal vein, which breaks y oa ene into capillaries, or, to speak more accurately, pseudo-capillaries, in the liver. The blood, rhether ¢ >From Spleen Stomach & Intestine Biliary ductule isouc0cscocoa0d20002 ROLLEI ELE = COGENT TARO EH 0690900959900500003G0'. DooNDODOCOO OD 00D00U!|. Booooad000GOOU00Oy) cCo0n0aC! ned ed Cees end bat Fig. 7.—DIAGRAM OF A LOBULE OF THE LIVER DIVIDED VERTICALLY THROUGH ITS AXIS. In its centre is a space, the intralobular vein, through which the blood falls into a branch of the hepatic vein, on its way to the heart. An interlobular branch of the portal vein, which brings the blood from the digestive organs, pours it by many smaller vessels over the surface of the lobule. It filters into the lebule through innumerable pseudo-capillary vessels, or spaces, between the radiating columns of liver-cells. Arterial blood is brought to the lobule by a twig of the hepatic artery. Bile is drained away from it by an affluent of the hepatic duct. In the lower part of the diagram seven liver-cells are shown, forming a divided column, magnified about 300 diameters. The cells are loaded with glycogen, and contain minute globules of fat. Red blood-corpuscles and two leucocytes are seen between the columns of liver-cells. One of the leucocytes has ingested two blood-corpuscles. ty ee a a ee ee ‘THE LIVER ages | ; ae away by the hepatic veins. The plan of structure of the liver is best understood when viewed with reference to ; Pths hepatic veins. These, if traced backwards, are found to break up into fairly straight vessels, each of which has a large number of lateral branches. Each of the lateral branches is in the centre of a mass of cells, which are packed round it in radiating columns. These masses, which have a_ diameter of from 1 to 2 millimetres,-are termed ‘“‘ lobules.”’ By mutual pressure the lobules are squeezed into a pentagonal or hexagonal form. The vein in the centre of the lobule is the intralobular vein. Turn now to the portal vein ; this is seen to break up into branches which run between the lobules, and are therefore termed “ interlobular veins.”” The branches of the hepatic artery also run between the lobules, as do the radicles of the bile-duct. Each lobule is a liver in miniature. The blood of the portal vein, which has come from the spleen, in which red blood-corpuscles are destroyed, and from the stomach and intestines, from which it has absorbed the pro- ducts of digestion, is poured over the surface of the lobule, to be filtered through into its central intralobular vein. In its passage from the interlobular veins (and branches of the hepatic artery) to the intralobular vein the blood is confined to radiating capillary channels ; but since these merely prevent the escape of red blood-corpuscles without imposing any restrictions upon the exudation of blood-plasma, the portal blood is to all intents and purposes filtered through the columns of liver-cells. The body-substance of the liver-cells is soft, destitute of envelope, and capable, when free on the (warmed) stage of a microscope, of changing in form, somewhat after the manner of a leucocyte. Such cells have a great capacity for taking up the products of digestion. Possibly they take up and store fats and pro- teins, but undoubtedly it is their chief business to absorb sugar which accumulates as glycogen in their substance. The glycogen is handed out to the hepatic blood as required. The pigment which results from the disintegration of red blood-corpuscles in the spleen is secreted, along with the bile- salts, into minute channels, or canaliculi, which groove the flat surfaces of adjacent liver-cells. These canaliculi converge to the bile-ducts. The liver is therefore at the same time the storehouse of sugar which it takes up from tke blood when 11 162 THE BODY AT WORK 3 it is in excess, and passes out to the blood when it is deficient, a and an excretory organ which eliminates the refuse of hemo- __ globin. The iron derived from hemoglobin it stores, and returns to the blood. ) Another function of the liver has been referred to already. It is the organ, and, as far as we know, the only organ, in © which urea is made in mammals, and uric acid in birds. If the liver of a freshly killed animal be excised and a stream of blood passed through it, the blood which leaves the organ contains urea. If a salt of ammonia, even the carbonate, be added to the blood, it is converted by the liver into urea. When a bird’s liver is made the subject of the same experi- ment, uric acid appears instead of urea. The liver can convert many nitrogenous substances into urea, but it seems probable that, normally, the salt with which it has chiefly to deal is lactate of ammonia (cf. p. 146). A few words must be added with regard to the functions of the liver during prenatal life, obscure though these functions are. The liver develops very early, and attains a relatively enormous size. At the third month it weighs as much as the whole of the rest of the body (cf. p. 34). Yet it cannot, one must suppose, have to do much of the work which falls to its share in postnatal life. Food is reaching the embryo in a constant stream, and not as the result of intermittent meals. The embryo has no need to store glycogen ; nor does its liver, on analysis, yield much of this substance. In the embryo glyco- gen is widely distributed throughout the tissues, not specially accumulated in the liver. No digestion is occurring in the alimentary canal. Bile is not needed to aid the hydrolysis and absorption of fats. A small quantity of cholesterin and less lecithin is being eliminated, but not much bile is needed to facilitate this process. A process which is proceeding at a great rate in the embryo, in various situations, is the formation of red blood-corpuscles. In this the liver takes part. But its duty in regard to blood- formation is not sufficiently onerous to account for its size. The formation of blood-corpuscles in the liver is observed with difficulty in microscopic sections. It is therefore impossible to speak with certainty as to the extent to which it is going on, but it may be safely asserted that this function by itself cannot FT. Zi y account for the great size of the organ in embryonic What other office it fills at this period is a question vhich still awaits an answer. There is no more curious chapter in medical history than the _ story of the views held at various periods with regard to the _ functions of the liver. From being a mere mass of “ paren- chyma ”’ serving as packing for the abdominal viscera, it was elevated to the rank of Grand Purifier of the “‘ humours ” of the body. Next, its excessive activity became the cause of that form of dyspepsia known as “ biliousness.”’ Still later its want of activity was its chief vice. A “sluggish ”’ liver was held responsible for mental perversity and moral dulness. Calomel, podophyllin, and other drugs were used as whips to stir it up ; and the increased secretions of the alimentary canal were mistaken for bile. Poor patient organ! It is the still- room of the body, in which the day’s supplies are stored, and from which they are served out, without haste and without delay. And it makes urea. What else it does we have yet to find out ; and it is not impossible that when physiologists have quite shaken themselves free from the explanations based upon conjecture, which their predecessors have handed down, they may discover that it has other duties which are not obvious, but of great importance. 11—2 . CHAPTER VII RESPIRATION Lire means change. We cannot imagine its continuance with- out liberation of energy. Arrest of molecular activity is death. There is no possibility of its revival. A watch that has stopped may be started by shaking. On the cessation of molecular activity an animate being becomes inanimate. Dead, it is liable to further chemical changes. Bacteria invade it. They shake down its complex unstable compounds into simple, stable, so-called “‘inorganic groups”’; but the ordered combination with oxygen, which constitutes living,can never recommence. Putre- faction may be prevented by the exclusion of germs. The inanimate mass of organic material may remain unchanged. Its return to life would be a miracle. From time to time a frog is found enclosed in old red sandstone, or some other rock which for countless ages has lain beneath the surface. The cleft through which the frog entered a few hours or days before it was discovered is overlooked. It is supposed to have lived ‘in a state of suspended animation ”’ for millions of years. The fact that no frogs are to be found among the fossils of the old red sandstone is an objection too casuistical to be seriously entertained. The physiologist’s demand to know what has become of the mountains of solid carbonic acid, water, and urea which the frog must have produced during its unimaginable term of incarceration is regarded as the natural expression of his prejudice—that life cannot continue without molecular change. And he is bound to admit his inability to prove that it cannot. Nevertheless, his experience that, whenever and however he may, by experimental methods, arrest change, he loses the power of causing it to recommence justifies him in his conviction that life is change. Even a living seed is to his mind an organism whose complex constituents are slowly— 164 / SAMDENTE MED EOC PORONTO | aunty. "RESPIRATION. however slowly—setting free energy by settling down the steps — __ which lead to stability and ultimate, inanimate rest ; and the only source of this energy is combination with oxygen. In the case of a seed the oxygen need not come from without. Seeds retain their power of germination after long occlusion in 3 nitrogen or other neutral gases. But all the time some change ____ ig occurring, some internal oxidation which resolves their less stable into more stable compounds. Otherwise they would not be alive. A physiologist is willing to believe that this may continue for ten years, fifteen years—for any period that the botanist tells him that he has, under verifiable conditions, observed that it does occur; but when he is told that peas taken from the hand of an Egyptian mummy, or seeds set free by the spades of navvies after a far longer burial, have been found to retain their vitality, his credulity is stretched beyond breaking-point. He cannot imagine a change so slow as to be spread over a geological period, still without exhaustion of all changeable compounds. The term “respiration”? has been extended until it is synonymous with “ oxidation.”’ At one time it was supposed that the combination of oxygen with oxidizable substances occurred in the lungs. The lungs were the hearth of the body, to which the blood brought fuel which burned in the air drawn into them. When it was understood that the actual com- bination of combustible material with oxygen occurs, not in the lungs, but in the tissues, a somewhat illogical distinction was made between “ external respiration ’’—the combination of oxygen and blood in the lungs—and “‘ internal respiration ”’ —the combination of oxygen and tissue-substances. The terms are not comparable. The taking up of oxygen by the hemoglobin of blood is. a different process to the union of oxygen, after the hemoglobin has parted with it, with the carbon, hydrogen, and nitrogen of the tissue-substances. The blood-stream carries both fuel and oxygen to the tissues, but the fuel while in the blood is not in an oxidizable con- dition. The foods are taken up by the tissues. They enter into combination with their protoplasm. Oxygen also com- _bines with tissue-substances. In proportion as the tissues are active oxidized compounds are split off. They fall into the lymph, whence they are absorbed by the blood. If they are a) CS Ct Nis STS EOS ee : ae ans Ailigg CVUCKOY poe, gone an Genme nitrogenous compounds, they are carried to the liver, forme od into urea, and passed to the kidneys for elimination. If car- — bonic acid, it is carried to the lungs for exhalation. The water formed by combination of hydrogen and oxygen may escape __ from the lungs, the kidneys, or the skin. Two or three pounds of mixed foods are consumed every day. . By the blood they are carried to the tissues, whence an equiva- lent quantity of waste—that is to say, oxidized—material is F removed. About 14 pounds of oxygen is required to burn the day’s fuel. The problems of respiration are twofold. In the first place we have to consider the physics and chemistry of the combina- tion of hemoglobin with oxygen, and of the elimination of carbonic acid from the blood in the lungs ; secondly we have to explain the transference of oxygen from hemoglobin to the tissues, and the reception in the blood of carbonic acid pro- duced by the tissues. The apparatus by which air is brought into relation with the blood consists of lungs and windpipe. At its upper end, where it joins the portion of the alimentary tract common to deglutition and respiration, the special respiratory tube is protected by the larynx. The nasal chambers belong to the respiratory tract; the gullet, or pharynx, is common to the two functions. The mucous membrane which lines the nose and windpipe is kept moist in order that it may catch particles of dust drawn in with the air. At the same time the nasal chambers serve to warm the air, and to add moisture to it if it be too dry; for the lining epithelium of the lungs would suffer if dry air came in contact with it. The wall-surface of the nasal chambers is increased by the projection of folded and chambered “turbinate bones.”” The importance of warming the air before it is admitted to the lungs is remarkably illus- trated in the case of certain sea-birds. The nasal chambers of the frigate-bird, and of some other birds which resemble it, are exceptionally complicated. Since the animal is devoid of any sense of smell, and the air which it breathes must be nearly satu- rated with moisture, the only function which can be assigned to these convoluted passages is that of warming inspired air. The larynx will be more minutely described when it is con- eae RESPIRATION — 167 __ sidered as the organ of voice. In connection with respiration, - it must be regarded as primarily a valve which closes the entrance to the windpipe during swallowing. It is overhung by a leaf-like appendage—the epiglottis—formed of exceed- ingly elastic tissue. It was thought until lately that the epiglottis drops over the aperture of the larynx when food is . passing down the gullet, and springs up again as soon as the act of deglutition is over; but recent observations have shown that during deglutition the epiglottis is pressed against the back of the tongue, and that the closure of the larynx is effected by its own sphincter muscles. The mucous membrane of the larynx is extremely sensitive to stimulation by anything which would be prejudicial to the tissue of the lungs. When its sensory nerve—the superior laryngeal—is stimulated, the larynx closes. It is the agent in carrying out many reflex actions, in which not the larynx only, but also the muscles of the chest and diaphragm, take part. For example, it imme- diately stops inspiration if an irritating vapour is present in the air. It stops respiration if any foreign body, such as a crumb of bread or a drop of water, touches the mucous membrane. When the trunk of the nerve is stimulated by an electric current, respiration is inhibited. Further, under suit- able stimulation the nerve brings about respiratory move- ments in which inspiration is gentle and expiration sudden, violent, convulsive. Rib-muscles and diaphragm combine to produce a cough, which ejects the noxious body. Again, its. stimulation in a different way probably helps to produce con- striction of the smaller bronchi which regulate the amount of air supplied to the air-cells of the lungs; although this con- striction may be largely due to a reflex which starts in the air- cells. The epithelium of the air-cells has an immensely rich supply of sensory nerves. In some persons this protective mechanism is very prone to overact its part. A little dust or foul gas in the air leads to such marked contraction of the bronchi that respiration becomes very difficult. Such an exaggerated tendency to reflex action constitutes the neurosis, asthma. In this malady the mechanism is unduly sensitive. Very slight stimulation leads to a maximum discharge of impulses to the muscular tissue of the bronchi. The trachea has a length of about 4 inches.. It extends from the neck beneath the = eal eves hus ge jor oe : under side of the arch of the aorta, where it divides into there right and left bronchi. The epithelium which lines the trachea — and .bronchi is ciliated. The cilia propel the secretion which — accumulates on its surface upwards towards the larynx. The a wall of the windpipe is kept open by rings of cartilage which are incomplete behind, where the trachea and cesophagus are in contact. Rings and plates of cartilage also support the bronchi. The bronchi divide and subdivide until their diameter is reduced to about 0-2 millimetre. Each bronchiole then breaks up into a bunch of very thin-walled, elongated infundibula, club-shaped, and with a diameter about five times that of the bronchiole with which they are connected. They may be three or four times as long as they are broad. The wall of an infundibulum is pitted like a piece of honeycomb into shallow chambers—the air-cells or alveoli. The walls of the air-chambers, or alveoli, are formed of a membrane upon which is spread a network of capillary blood- vessels. The air-chambers are so closely packed together that a common wall separates one chamber from the next adjoining. Minute bloodvessels pierce the partitions which separate the chambers, appearing now on one side of the wall, now on the other. The air-chambers are lined by thin epithelial scales or tiles. The blood in the capillary vessels is separated from the air in the air-chambers by the wall of the capillary; by a lymph-space, probably rather potential than actual; and by the epithelial tiles. This covering suffices to prevent the escape both of red corpuscles and of plasma, yet offers very little resistance to the passage of gases from the blood into the air, and from the air into the blood. Leucocytes make their way between the tiles, and creep over their internal surfaces, searching for cell débris or foreign matter. Anything that they find they carry to the clumps of lymphoid tissue which occur in the outer wall of the bronchi. In a town-dweller, leucocytes are found in these lymph- thickets, charged with particles of soot. They show droplets of fat and other evidences of degeneration. At other spots are to be seen little collections of soot which have been left behind after the dissolution of the leucocytes which brought them there. io EMR See ee Nicer, RE i. fost ee) a 5 Ss RESPIRATION 169 It is not possible to make anything like an accurate estimate of the number of alveoli in the lungs ; 725,000,000 is a figure arrived at by measuring the average cubic capacity of an alveolus, and comparing it with the total cubic capacity of the lungs. Each alveolus supports some forty or fifty capillary vessels. The superficial area of vascular membrane exposed is placed at 90 square metres, or about 100 times the area of the skin. Figures such as these convey very little meaning, but they help one to realize the magnitude of the provision made for the aeration of the blood. Pneumonia is a condition in which the lining of the air- chambers is inflamed, usually, possibly always, owing to the entrance of bacteria. Lymph exudes through the walls of the alveoli. Epithelial scales flake off. Pus-cells (dead leucocytes) accumulate in the air-chambers. Respiration is curtailed, and dyspnoea results. After a time, if the case progresses favour- ably, “ resolution,” as it is technically termed, begins to occur. The exuded substances are either expectorated or absorbed, and the lung-tissue returns to a normal condition. Here a few words may be devoted to respiratory sounds. Spirare means to sigh. Breathing received the name by which it is known in physiology from the sound which accompanies the exit of air from the nostrils. Since the introduction of auscultation as a means of ascertaining the condition of the lungs, other sounds, not heard until the ear or a stethoscope is placed against the chest, have acquired great importance. These sounds, termed “‘ murmurs,” may be divided into two classes. (a) When the ear is placed against the windpipe, or in the middle of the back between the shoulder-blades, a murmur is audible, due to the movement of air through the larynx. If the larynx, the trachea, or the bronchi contain mucus, it is a harsh, rough, bubbling, or crackling sound. It accompanies both inspiration and expiration. (b) A softer, more delicate murmur is heard when the ear is placed against the front or the side of the chest. This is the vesicular or pulmonary murmur. It is heard during inspiration, and is due to the passage of air out of the smallest bronchi into the more spacious infundibula in which they end. These two kinds of murmur must be rigidly distinguished—the laryngeal murmur, heard in situations in which no lung-tissue interveres between the Ae he Ne ee Le ee Te et on fe, ae ie ne eS Seg eben goat tett et Te he ; : Pee Pall ion < Da ee aa 170 | THE BODY AT WORK ear and the great tracheal or bronchial tubes ; and the pul- a monary murmur, heard over all regions where the bronchi are buried in lung. Healthy lung is as bad a conductor of sound as a sponge or a wad of cotton-wool. The laryngeal murmur is inaudible in regions in which lung lies beneath the chest-wall. It would be far beyond the scope of this book to attempt to describe the very varied alterations in the chest-_ sounds which may be produced by disease. The student would do well to familiarize himself with the nature of the sounds which are heard in health, and the situations in which they are heard, in order that he may be able, in abnormal conditions, to recognize that something is wrong. The chief departures from the normal may be grouped under the following heads : (1) The pulmonary murmur may lose its soft, smooth, sighing character owing to inflammation of the alveoli and infundibula. It may be as loud in expiration as in inspiration. Only a practised ear can estimate the signifi- cance of these changes. (2) The laryngeal murmur may be reinforced by “rales ’’—a convenient term for supplementary sounds. The source of such rales may be a cold in the chest, laryngitis, or bronchitis of various degrees. (3) The laryngeal murmur may be heard in situations in which lung intervenes between the ear and the larger bronchial tubes. This can be due only to the lung being in an abnormal condition as a con- ductor ef sound. Instead of being as spongy as well-made Vienna bread, its air-spaces are filled with solid or fluid deposit. It is as firm as dough. To such a condition it attains at the height of pneumonia—a stage termed “ hepatization ”’ because in section it looks like liver rather than lung. Breathing is the enlargement and diminution of the chest, which causes air to be drawn into and expressed from the lungs. The windpipe being open, the air inside the lungs is, of course, at the same pressure as the atmosphere. Expansion of the chest results in the equal expansion of the lungs. Since there is no air-space between the outer surface of the lungs and the inner surface of the chest-wall, the lungs cannot separate from the chest-wall when it expands. But the lungs contain elastic tissue always slightly on the stretch. If the chest be punctured, and air admitted between the chest-wall and the lungs, the lungs collapse. The expiratory movement, RESPIRATION 171 the contraction of the chest, is due to the elasticity of the lungs. This tendency on the part of the lungs to contract is _ sufficient in quiet respiration to restore the chest to its usual size after inspiration, and thus to expel air. The lungs are held open owing to the negative pressure in the space which separates them from the chest-wall. This negative pressure has a most important relation to their permeability by air. Imagine the condition reversed. Picture a lung into which air is forced by a muscular pump. After each stroke of the pump the lung would collapse. Its finest tubes and their dilated terminations could be maintained as open spaces, between the strokes of the pump, only by giving a consider- able thickness and firmness to their walls. Such a substantial structure would be unfavourable to an interchange of gases between the blood and the air. The reverse of this condition is found in Nature. The lung is stretched from without. Its tissue, delicate as crépe, cannot collapse even at the end of the deepest expiration. The ribs are united by intercostal muscles, disposed in two sheets. The fibres of the external intercostals are directed downwards and forwards, those of the internal intercostals downwards and backwards. In tranquil respiration the chest is enlarged by the external intercostal muscles, which raise the ribs, and the diaphragmatic muscle, which renders peripheral portions of the diaphragm flat. The role of the internal inter- costal muscles is a subject still under discussion. For the most part, physiologists regard them as accessory to expira- tion, but some hold that they combine with the external inter- costals in raising the ribs and twisting them outwards during inspiration. The diaphragm is a partition which separates the thoracic from the abdominal cavity. It is in the form of a vault. The central portion of the dome is membranous, its margins muscular. Its membranous centre is in contact with the pericardium, which encloses the heart. The level of this part is therefore fixed, except in forced inspiration, when it descends slightly. It constitutes a fixed plane for the muscles of the diaphragm, which are attached below to the vertebral column and the ribs. When the muscles contract in inspira- tion, the curvature of the marginal portions of the diaphragm is diminished, and the chest-cavity consequently enlarged. Speaae SE ‘the space here aha 3 diaphragm and the chest-wall closes up, and the lower | b of the lung slips out of it. There is a marked difference in the relaiive extent of one a costal and diaphragmatic movements in men and women. In — Fa women respiration is chiefly costal; in men it is chiefly diaphragmatic. In men the abdomen moves forwards, as the ql 7 diaphragm descends in tranquil breathing ; in women the chest ' rises. Men who wish, for the purposes of athletics, or singing, A B iagay Right Ventricle - Rib v(? x88 fo +} Mid line of body < Fig. 8.—THE DIAPHRAGM AND ORGANS IN CONTAOT WITH IT—A, IN EXPIRATION ; B, AT THE END OF A DEEP INSPIRATION. TRANSVERSE VERTICAL SECTIONS IN THE LINE OF THE ARMPIT. A, At the end of an ordinary expiration the lung does not extend below the upper border of the eighth rib. From this level to the middle or lower border of the tenth rib the two layers of the pleura covering respectively the inner wall of the chest and the upper surface of the diaphragm are in contact. B, When the lung is distended with air it occupies the whole of the pleural cavity. or public speaking, to retain the power of making the most of their chest-capacity are wise in not allowing themselves to fall into the habit of lazy, abdominal breathing. When additional pressure is required, when respiration is forced, various external muscles attached to the spinal column, the shoulder-blades, and the clavicles, as well as the muscles of the abdomen, come into play. The chest is lined and the lungs covered by a serous mem- brane—the pleura. Normally there is only just sufficient lymph in the space between the visceral layer of the pleura eee ee ae ee ego A Pree pre er ' = z - . ve — - ORESPIRATION 1B which invests the lungs and the parietal layer which lines the chest-wall to prevent friction during respiration. When the pleura is inflamed, one layer of the membrane rubs against the other. In the early or “dry ” stage of pleurisy, the physician recognizes this condition by the friction-sound which he hears on placing his stethoscope against the chest. In a later stage lymph (pleuritic fluid) is poured out. It accumulates in the lower part of the chest, and is recognized by the absence of the resonant note which, under normal conditions, is given out by the chest when percussed. The lungs are not compressed during expiration ; they are not squeezed, as a pair of bellows or a sponge may be squeezed, emptying it of its contents. At the end of tranquil expiration the lungs still contain about 34 litres of air. At the top of tranquil inspiration the volume of their contents does not exceed 4 litres. It is evident, therefore, that air is not drawn into and driven out from the air-chambers by the movements of respiration. The tide of air does not extend far beyond the ends of the bronchi. The gases in the air-chambers are ex- changed with the fresh air drawn into the infundibula by diffusion. The composition of the air which is in contact with the bloodvessels is constant. It is about 4 per cent. poorer in oxygen and 3 per cent. richer in carbonic acid than the outside air. Of the air drawn into the windpipe during an inspiration, about one-third returns to the open with the following expira- tion ; two-thirds remains in the lungs. If, therefore, the air taken in at each tide equals one-seventh of the quantity already in the lungs, and if of this one-seventh two-thirds remains, each alveolus renews about one-tenth of its air. Its contents are completely changed in ten respirations. Fresh air is composed of 21 per cent. oxygen, 79 per cent. nitrogen, and a trace (0-04 per cent.) of carbonic acid. Forced by a syringe through lime-water, fresh air does not produce any appreciable milkiness, whereas air breathed through a tube into lime-water renders it turbid owing to the formation of carbonate of lime. Carbonic acid (CO,) occupies the same volume as its oxygen (O,) would occupy if free. The oxygen which breathed air has lost slightly exceeds in amount the carbonic acid which it has gained in exchange. The differ- ence is due to the retention of some of the oxygen for the pur- * ; pose of uniting with hydrogen to form water, and of forming urea. The proportion between carbonic acid gained and ¢ CO, . ¢ . * 29 oxygen lost, 0,’ 8 termed the “respiratory quotient.” Its 2 value varies, of course, with diet. In a herbivorous animal, whose food consists of carbohydrates, it departs but little from unity; in a carnivore, which eats fat and nitrogen- containing food, it is about 0-8. The respiratory exchange is very much smaller in cold- blooded animals than in animals which maintain the tempera- ture of the body at a fixed level. In warm-blooded animals it rises as the temperature falls, falls as it rises, the increased oxidation warming the body, the diminished oxidation allow- ing it to cool; whereas in cold-blooded animals it increases as the temperature rises, owing to the greater activity induced by warmth, and falls as the temperature falls. The respiratory exchange is increased by muscular activity. If the amounts of oxygen absorbed and carbonic acid given out are measured while a man is at rest, and again while he is doing hard physical work, it is found that during work the respiratory exchange is twice as great as during rest. During periods of starvation the respiratory exchange remains un- altered, since heat has to be constantly produced if the tem- perature of the body is to be kept from falling. Since the purpose of respiration is to give to the blood the opportunity of renewing its supply of oxygen, and of getting rid of the carbonic acid with which it is charged, it might be supposed that the respiratory exchange would be increased, so far as the intake of oxygen is concerned, by breathing oxygen gas instead of air; but it appears that under normal conditions nothing is gained. When an animal is breathing air, its blood takes up all the oxygen that it wants—all the oxygen, that is to say, for which its tissues are asking. Offering it pure oxygen in place of mixed oxygen and nitrogen does not induce it totake up more. The hemoglobin is almost saturated with oxygen when the blood leaves the lungs under ordinary conditions. In certain diseases of the lungs, however, in which the blood becomes unduly venous, the respiration of oxygen may be beneficial ; but even in these cases the results are dis- -- RESPIRATION 175 BS appointing, because the system is suffering much less from = deficiency of oxygen than from accumulation of carbonic acid. Substituting oxygen for air does not facilitate the escape of carbonic acid. — The nervous mechanism of respiration has been the subject of much investigation and ‘of many experiments, without, it must be confessed, the development of a quite complete or satisfactory theory. Rospiration is a rhythmic process. About seventeen times in a minute the intercostal and dia- phragmatic muscles contract. Inspiration is immediately followed by expiration, the falling movement being due, as already explained, to the elasticity of the lungs, which are stretched during inspiration. A slight pause intervenes between the end of expiration and the commencement of the next inspiratory movement. ‘Tranquil respiration is a succes- sion of reflex inspiratory movements, the depth of which varies according to the needs of the body—that is to say, according to the condition of the blood. If the need for aeration of the blood becomes urgent, the depth of inspiration is increased, and expiration also becomes an active movement, certain muscles, especially those of the abdomen, being called into play. In this condition two sets of reflex actions alternate. A large number of nerves are concerned even in tranquil respiration. If a man in falling “ breaks his back ”’ at the junction of the cervical and thoracic regions, costal respiration ceases. The series of intercostal nerves which arises from the dorsal spinal cord below the level at which it is injured are thrown out of action. Diaphragmatic respiration still continues, because the nerve of the diaphragm, the phrenic, arises from cervical roots. The lungs are supplied by the vagus nerve. This nerve joins the medulla oblongata as one of a group of three—glosso- pharyngeal, vagus, and spinal accessory—which by a large number of roots enter the groove between the olive and the restiform body. ‘The vagus is the channel along which afferent impulses from the lungs enter the medulla. Such impulses call for respiratory movements. Cutting both vagi, however, does not put an end to respiration. Inspiratory movements continue, but they are much deeper and separated by much longer pauses. Such a form of respiration is inefficient. The blood is not properly aerated. The animal fells into a condi- ts inc i Eee ieee ei LM 176 THE BODY AT WORK tion of dyspnoea, which ends in death. When the central end of the cut vagus is stimulated, the movements become more > natural. Clearly, the respiratory reflex is not dependent upon the vagus, since it continues after the nerve is cut, although the impulses which pass up this nerve regulate its rhythm. They govern the length of the inspiratory move- ments, cut them short at the right moment, and secure their succession at proper intervals. The transfer of afferent impulses into efferent channels occurs in the medulla oblongata. Long ago it was found that if the brain above this level be removed, part by part, respira- tion is not interfered with until the medulla oblongata is injured. When a cut is made into the floor of the fourth ventricle not far to one side of the middle line, the respiratory movements on that side of the body cease. If the injury be bilateral, even though very limited in extent, respiration stops. This spot was therefore spoken of as the “ respiratory centre.” Flourens, who first discovered it, believed that it was a mere spot. He gave to it the fanciful name of neud vital. It is the place at which the afferent nerves which call for respiration are brought into connection with all the various motor nerves which bring about the respiratory movements of nostrils, larynx, chest, and diaphragm. Possibly the knife in Flourens’ incision divides the tract of fibres which distri- butes afferent impulses, but whether the junction be a defined tract or no, injury to this region of the medulla throws the nervous mechanism of respiration out of gear. At this par- ticular spot lies the “centre ”’ for respiration—the one part of the nervous system which must be intact if the movements of respiration are to be carried out. There is no reason for thinking that respiratory impulses are generated at this spot. It is a centre in the same sense in which Crewe is a centre for distributing the goods of Lancashire and other parts of England to North Wales. The use of the term “ nerve-centre ”’ has been very much abused. Centres were supposed to be collections of cells, each group of which had some prerogative of initiation. Reasoning from the analogy of human institutions, it was thought necessary that the nervous system should be organized into departments severally responsible for the administration of the activities of certain sets of muscles : one centre controlled 177 The centres were dependent one on another; each regulated lower centres, and was governed by those above it, in this bureaucratic scheme. We know nothing of any function of nerve-cells other than that of transmitting impulses. All that we know about nerve-cells is that they place afferent and efferent routes in communication, and interpose resistance into nerve-circuits. Every nerve-cell of the grey matter of the brain and spinal cord gives off processes which ramify. The ultimate twigs into which a branch divides are in connection with other sets of twigs derived from the end-branchings of nerve-fibres or processes of other nerve-cells. A nerve-fibre is but the axis-cylinder process of a nerve-cell. Impulses en- counter resistance in passing along the neuro-fibrillee (cf. Fig. 22) contained in the twig-connections of the ramifying processes of nerve-cells. There is no reason for supposing that anything like the same resistance is offered to the passage of impulses along the fibrillee where they lie within the stout branches of the cell- processes or within the body of the cell. It is easy to make a pictorial representation of such a mechanism. Imagine a model of the stem of a tree made by binding together a large number of wires ; its branches as containing small groups of wires ; the ultimate twigs as separate wires. Carry wires from the roots of one tree to the branches of another. Trees so constructed might be taken as representing nerve-cells. We have not as yet succeeded in demonstrating the isolated neuro- fibrille as they pass over from the end-twigs of a nerve-fibre to the end-twigs of a nerve-cell branch, but we have abundant reason for believing that they do so pass, and that the resistance to the passage of a nerve-impulse is interposed in this neutral or junctional zone. This resistance has to be overcome. It is overcome by the summation of impulses. All nerve- impulses are vibratory. The first vibrations fail may to get through ; but if the vibrations continue, they exert a cumula- tive effect. After a time they overcome the resistance ; sen- sory impulses flow through the centre into motor channels. In this way we endeavour to explain the rhythmic discharge through the respiratory and other centres. It has not been found possible to determine the source of all the afferent impulses which reach the centre. Respiration continues after 12 - eele lee Qe Ss Pcie Se eT ee LE ro ge SE ee eet me Oe te ee 7-1 . 7 I ea +) in 7 pny ‘ ae v bs am . UP aimee e = bd hb ee 8 na % ‘ t + Le. i mae < ~ . a . at . 178 all accessible nerves have been cut, including even the posterior Fe roots of the cervical nerves. Probably it is a mistake to look for definite afferent channels in the medulla and the rest of the brain. All parts of the body need aerated blood. From all parts, including nerve-tissue itself, arises the demand for respiration. Possibly nerve-centres have the power, as it were, of storing impulses, and discharging them after the stream of fresh arrivals has ceased to flow. They may acquire a habit. The resistance in the centre is profoundly affected by the condition of the blood. As the blood becomes more venous, impulses pass across the nerve connections with ever-increasing force. Kept in the first instance to definite channels, they spread as the centre becomes more excitable farther and farther afield, reaching one group of muscles after another, and pressing them into the service of respiration. When, in dyspnoea, every muscle which can in any way help the move- ments of the chest is doing its best, others which are useless for this purpose receive the reflected impulses and join in, producing general convulsions. The increased activity of the respiratory centre which is produced by slight venosity of the blood is shown in the rapid and deep inspirations which are caused by violent exercise. Perhaps it is justifiable to go a step farther, and to assert that there is something in blood which has been rendered venous by muscular activity which is specially exciting to the respiratory centre. If the blood from a limb be prevented from returning to the general cir- culation, by compressing or tying its great veins, and if the muscles of the limb be strongly stimulated by an electric current, their activity, so long as the passage through the veins is blocked, has no influence upon respiration. But, on relaxation of the pressure on the veins, respiration may become twice as deep and twice as frequent as it was before the muscles were stimulated, although the limb is now in a condition of perfect rest. What is the special action of the vagus nerve ? Its superior larnygeal branch checks inspiration and induces expiration, as already said. The impulses which pass up its main trunk bring about ordered movements. They are not dependent for their generation upon the condition of the blood in the lungs. When the chest is filled with nitrogen, inspiration and expira- ee RESPIRATION — a tion alternate in the usual way, although the blood is growing _ steadily more venous. The failure of inspiration to bring about aeration of the blood does not lead to a prolongation of the inspiratory effort. Inspiration is cut off and expiration estab- lished in regular sequence. In performing “ artificial respira- _ tion”? (cf. p. 184) for the purpose of saving life, in cases in which respiration has ceased owing to the lungs being filled with water, or for other reasons, the chest is enlarged by raising the arms above the head, and diminished by pressing the elbows against the sides. Enlargement promotes a tendency to ex- piration, compression a tendency to a natural inspiratory effort. Evidently there is a connection between the movements of the chest and the stimulation of the respiratory centre. If respira- tion is being carried on artificially, by forcing air from a bellows into the trachea, the nostrils dilate as the chest is distended, and contract as it is emptied, so long as the vagus nerve is intact, just as they do in normal respiration. This shows that, when the chest is emptied, a message is sent through to the nucleus of origin of the nerve which supplies the dilator muscles of the nostril. When the lungs are full, a message calls upon the nostrils to contract. The only factor which is common to pressing in and pulling out the ribs, and filling and exhausting the lungs with a bellows, is the alteration in the form of the lungs which is produced by the two methods. It is impossible to resist the conclusion that the stretching of the tissue of the lungs stimulates the nerve-endings of the vagus. The impulses thus induced automatically stop inspiration, and lead to an expiratory effort. There are many indications that the nervous mechanism of respiration is a double one, certain stimuli inducing expiration, with inhibition of inspiration, others inhibiting expiration and inducing inspiration. There are, however, many difficulties in the way of formulating a satisfactory theory of the relation of these antagonistic actions. We may frequently observe indi- cations of such an antagonism between the two phases of the respiratory mechanism. Cold water dashed on the back of the head (when the head is being shampooed) induces a long in- spiration with inhibition of expiration. A blow in the pit of the stomach “ knocks all the wind out of a man.” Expiration is prolonged until the lungs are unusually empty, and yet the 12—2 180 “THE BODY AT WORK victim of the blow feels as if he would never again be able to — draw breath. : Modified Respiratory Movements.—The object of cought is to expel foreign matter from the windpipe or larynx; of — sneezing, to clear the nose. The former action consists of a long deep inspiration; the closure of the glottis; a forcible expiration. The blast of air encountering a closed glottis acquires considerable pressure. When the resistance of the glottis is overcome, the blast rushes through, carrying with it mucus or bread-crumb, or whatever the substance may be which irritated the endings of the superior laryngeal nerve. In sneezing, the back of the tongue is thrust against the palate, closing the aperture of the fauces. Inspiration is prolonged. A strong expiration follows. The blast rushes through the nasal cavities. This reflex is usually provoked by a tickling of the endings of the fifth nerve in the nasal mucous mem- brane. It is also caused in many persons, through the optic nerve, by a bright light ; an apparently purposeless reflex about which we shall have something more to say in a subsequent chapter. Laughing and crying are modified respiratory move- ments as useless, so far as any immediate purpose is accom- plished, as sneezing in response to a bright light. As means of expressing emotions they have been cultivated by the human race. Possibly a case for crying might be made out on physiological grounds. Under certain circumstances it relieves a feeling of distress which, while it lasts, is detrimental to the proper functions of the body. Laughing undoubtedly is beneficial. The rapid movements of the chest quicken the circulation. The shaking of the midriff favours the discharge of digestive secretions, accelerates the movements of the alimen- tary canal, and generally is beneficial to digestion. But “laugh and grow fat ” is not necessarily the order of cause and effect. An efficient digestion and a good capacity for assimila- tion lead to a sense of bien-étre which predisposes to a merry view of life. Yawning is a deep inspiration with open mouth and larynx. It commences usually at the end of a normal inspiration, a slight pause being followed by further inspiration, deep and prolonged. Its commencement seems to be due to impulses generated by the relaxation of the tone of the muscle which 181 a — Papen, pe the j nee to fall. A sehen Sibhsauties of the - muscles which open the mouth immediately follows. Muscles _ of the neck and head also come into play. Not improbably the yawn ends in a general stretch. If the origin of this reflex is obscure, its usefulness is marked. The circulation is _ quickened, the blood is changed, nervous system and muscles again become alert. “‘ Apnea ”’ is the condition of arrested respiration. If aman about to dive into the water breathe deeply and rapidly half a dozen times, he abolishes for a while the desire to breathe. One is naturally inclined to explain this as due to a surplus of oxygen taken into the blood, but a moment’s reflection shows that this cannot be the cause. In the first place, as we have already pointed out, the blood which leaves the lungs in tran- quil respiration is very nearly saturated with oxygen. It can take up but little more. Again, the deep inspirations do not change the air in the air-chambers; time is required for the renewal by diffusion of their gaseous contents. It is improbable that the constitution of the air in the alveoli is sensibly altered by a few deep breaths. Probably the explana- tion is to be found in the effect upon the nerve-centre of dis- tention of the chest. Stretching the nerve-endings of the vagus in the lungs inhibits inspiration. If the stimulation be excessive, inspiration is inhibited for a considerable time. That this is the right theory of apnea is proved by repeatedly inflating the lungs of an anesthetized animal with a pair of bellows. The same arrest of inspiration is induced whether the lungs are inflated with air or with a neutral gas, such as nitrogen, so long as the vagus nerve is intact. If this be cut, inflation with a neutral gas no longer produces apnea. ** Dyspnea ”’ is the term applied to the complex conditions and movements which result from deficient aeration of the blood, or, rather, from the distribution of insufficiently aerated blood to the centres in the medulla oblongata. The blood of the rest of the body may be in a satisfactory condition, but if, owing to ligature of the carotid and vertebral arteries or other causes, the blood supplied to the brain be inadequate to its proper nutrition, the phenomena of dyspnoea are as marked as they are when air is prevented from entering the lungs. Rite a hes. ass 182 THE BODY AT WORK _ That the excitability of the nerve-centres in the brain is greatly i increased when this organ is supplied with venous blood, and _ that their tendency to transmit impulses which call for respira- tion is consequently exaggerated, is remarkably shown by the _ Ee: following experiment: Two rabbits—A. and B.—are placed under the influence of chloroform. Their carotid arteries are cut, and a crossed circulation established by connecting the proximal ends of A.’s arteries with the distal ends of B.’s, and vice versa. The head of each rabbit is now supplied with blood from the heart of the other, the rest of its body by blood from its own heart. A.’s chest is now opened, so that its lungs collapse and cease to take part in respiration. The animal continues to make the movements of respiration in a tranquil manner, whereas B. is thrown into violent dyspnea. The animal whose brain is receiving aerated blood remains normal, notwithstanding the fact that its lungs and the rest of its body are poisoned with venous blood. The animal whose brain is supplied with venous blood becomes dyspneeic, although its lungs and body are receiving pure arterial blood. There is a regular sequence in the phenomena of dyspnoea leading up to the final stage termed “ asphyxia.” If the trachea be suddenly blocked, so that no air can pass, the respiratory movements at once become deeper and more rapid. This con- dition is termed “‘hyperpnea.” In a comparatively few seconds the system appears, as it were, to find out that inspira- tion is not needed. Expiratory efforts begin to preponderate. They increase in violence. All accessory muscles are brought into play. The cry for air is heard even by muscles which cannot help. Muscles of the limbs contract, although their con- traction has no effect upon the capacity of the chest. Every expiratory effort is accompanied by convulsions of a flexor type. At the end of two minutes there is usually a sudden change. Attempts at expiration cease. Slow, deep, infre- quent inspirations take their place, accompanied by convulsions of extensor muscles. Pupils are widely dilated, mouth open, head thrown back. The subject is absolutely insensitive to every kind of stimulus. The pulse shows a high arterial ten- sion. ‘The beating of the heart is slow and strong. In about four minutes from the time at which the windpipe was blocked respiratory movements cease. The arterial tension falls. The 183 heart’s action grows rapidly weaker, although for two or three minutes longer it may still continue to flicker. Recovery is possible until it finally gives up. After death the right side of the heart is found gorged with blood, the left side empty, showing that the heart had been unable to force the blood through the capillaries of the lungs. Under all ordinary conditions the sequence of phenomena of asphyxia is the same—a stage of exaggerated breathing (hyperpneea), a stage marked by the co-operation of muscles which are not called into action in tranquil breathing (dyspnea), followed by the condition of asphyxia properly so termed. An animal whose supply of fresh air is cut off passes through these three stages, whether it be enclosed in a small space or in a very large one. It must, however, be noted that in asphyxia several factors combine in varying degrees. Carbonic acid is in excess in the blood, oxygen deficient. The nervous mechanism which regulates respiratory movements is thrown out of gear. Motor and inhibitory impulses are in conflict. It is important, if these complex phenomena are to be analysed, that one factor only should be altered at any given time. For example, carbonic acid may be allowed to increase in the air while a constant oxygen-tension is maintained. Under these circumstances the dyspneeic contractions are much less marked. No convulsions follow. The paralysing action of carbonic acid predominates. Anesthesia passes into complete unconscious- ness. Death is tranquil. And this, speaking generally, is what happens in disease of the lungs. Asphyxia comes on slowly. The supply of oxygen is undiminished, but carbonic ~ acid accumulates in the blood, acting as a narcotic poison which lowers the excitability of the nervous system, suspends con- ~ sciousness, and slowly brings the vital activities to a standstill. In cases of drowning, when the lungs are filled with water, the resistance to the passage of blood through their capillary vessels is greater than it is when they are still filled with air. The heart is sooner beaten in its effort to drive the blood through them. Usually it stops in about four minutes. Yet it is difficult to say for how long after a person has been im- mersed in water it may be still possible to resuscitate him. Reports vary, owing in large measure to uncertainty as to the exact time at which the immersed person sank and his lungs See Xe, a filled with water. It is a wise precept that t artificial respiration “S should be tried in every case, without waiting a single instant __ to ascertain whether the heart still beats. The first thing to — do is to empty the chest of water. Then place the subject on his back. Kneel on the ground behind his head. Grasp an arm just below the elbow, in each hand. Draw the arms up above the patient’s head, so that the pectoral and other muscles drag on the ribs, enlarging the chest ; then lower them, and press them into the sides. This must be done with the natural rhythm of respiration, and not more frequently than twenty times in a minute. It is well if an assistant draws the tongue forward, to give free admission to air. Presumably the slight exchange of air brought about by mechanical expansion and compression of the chest favours the passage of blood through the capillaries of the lungs; but the real object of artificial respiration is to stretch the endings of the vagus nerve, and in this way to originate impulses which will call the respiratory centre into action. Perhaps it may not be super- fluous to point out that the failure of the pulse must not be taken as an indication that the heart has ceased to beat. Owing to the obstruction to the circulation through the lungs, the left side of the heart is almost empty. Very little blood is pumped into the aorta. None reaches the wrist. Exchange of Gases in the Lungs.—In the lungs each red corpuscle takes from the air a charge of oxygen which it carries to the tissues. In the tissues the plasma of the blood receives carbonic acid, which escapes from it when it reaches the lungs. Water dissolves oxygen and carbonic acid. Towards animals and plants which live in it, water plays the same role as the atmosphere towards dwellers on land. The quantity of a gas which will dissolve in water is proportional to the pressure to which it is subjected. If water were the circulating fluid, some oxygen would enter it in the lungs ; some carbonic acid would be taken up in the tissues and liberated in the lungs. But it is clear that the small quantity of fluid which the vascular system will hold would be incapable of serving as an efficient medium of exchange between the tissues and the lungs. When a given quantity of venous blood is agitated with air, five times as much oxygen is taken up as the blood could carry if the gas were simply dissolved. Both oxygen and ee ga Pe 5 Ms rem Ad, ane A r Cc acid. are held by the blood in chemical combina- a Z ULU he "The condition in which oxygen is carried was discovered in 1864 (cf. p. 68). From all time it had been noticed that the blood which flows from a vein is darker and of a more purple tint than the blood which spurts out of a cut artery. Shortly before the date mentioned above, the spectroscope had begun to be used to distinguish more accurately than the eye can do the groups of rays which a coloured solution transmits. The colour of a ray of light depends upon its wave-length. The light of the sun, when its rays are sorted by a prism, accord- ing to their wave-lengths, shows all colours from the long waves of red to the short rays of violet, with certain gaps. At intervals where rays are missing, the spectrum exhibits dark bands—Fraunhofer’s lines. The colour of a solution is measured by placing’a flat-sided vessel containing it in the course of a beam of the sun’s light, on its way to a prism. When the rays are spread. out, it is observed that certain groups have been absorbed by the coloured fluid. The colour of the solution is due to the rays which it transmits. It had been pointed out in 1862 that blood diluted with water absorbs parts of each end of the spectrum, and also two groups of rays lying between the fixed bands of Fraunhofer which spectroscopists had labelled D and E. Stokes observed that this is true only of arterial blood. Venous blood absorbs a broad band in this part of the spectrum in place of the two narrow bands. He showed that, “like indigo, it is capable of existing in two states of oxidation, dis- tinguishable by a difference of colour and a fundamental dif- ference in the action on the spectrum. It may be made to pass from the more to the less oxidized condition by the action of suitable reducing agents, and recovers its oxygen by absorp- tion from the air.”’ The reducing agents of which Stokes made use were alkaline solutions of ferrous sulphate or of stannous chloride containing some citric or tartaric acid. These sub- salts of iron and tin very rapidly absorb oxygen from the air or from any chemical substance which parts with it readily. With thes esolutions Stokes replaced the tissues. He abstracted the oxygen from the oxyhemoglobin; then, shaking the solution of reduced hemoglobin with air, he reprodnces the action which occurs in the lungs. ne Ae Se eee Merle a Se er ee a ol sey eS Fy ee ae A ey = ; - : ; - : . . od : ? “ ° r i = * ieee THE BODY AY WORK : If the hand be held between a spectroscope and the sonal 4 of light, in such a position that the beam passes through the thin tissue of two fingers where they are in contact, the spec- _ trum of oxyhemoglobin is obtained. If now the circulation through the fingers be impeded by putting strong indiarubber bands round them, the blood becomes venous, and the two narrow bands of oxyhemoglobin give cia to the broad band of reduced hemoglobin. q Although very soluble, hemoglobin may be obtained in | crystals, the form of which varies in different animals. When obtained from human blood, the crystals are rhombic prisms ; from the guinea-pig, tetrahedra ; from the squirrel, hexagonal plates. Yet it is unlikely that the hemoglobin of one animal differs chemically from that of another in any proper sense of the term. Probably the form of the crystals depends upon the amount of water of crystallization. The apparent polymorphism of hemoglobin may be associated with the great size of its molecules (cf. p. 66). Even when in the crystalline form, hemoglobin can take up oxygen ; but the difficulties which attend its purification and crystallization render somewhat uncertain the amount of oxygen which a gramme of crystallized hemoglobin can absorb. In solutior, 1 gramme can take up 1-34 cubic centimetres. The whole of the hemoglobin of the body would, therefore, if it were all in the oxidized condition, hold about 4 grammes of oxygen. It is not with oxygen alone that hemoglobin can combine. It can absorb the same volume of carbonic oxide or of nitric oxide gas. Both of these gases it holds more firmly than oxygen. Neither carbonic oxide-hemoglobin nor nitric oxide- hemoglobin is of any use to the tissues. If the blood becomes charged with the fumes of carbonic oxide (CO) given off by a coke-fire, this gas proves extremely poisonous. The blood does not lose it in its circuit through the body, nor is it exchanged for oxygen in the lungs. The instability of the compound of hemoglobin and oxygen is Shown under the air-pump. The pressure of air in the open equals 760 millimetres of mercury. When the pressure falls to about 250 millimetres, the oxygen is rapidly given off. This is a matter of considerable interest in its bearing upon the ques- : Fee ion | 187 - tion of the height to which it is possible for a human being to ascend. An animal placed in a chamber from which the air is pumped dies when the pressure falls to 250 millimetres of mercury. It has been ascertained that a man under the same circumstances can bear with impunity a reduction to 300 milli- metres. How much lower must the pressure fall before it proves fatal 2? Of three aeronauts who ascended in the balloon Zenith to a height of 8,600 metres (26,500 feet), two died. The third, Tissandier, became unconscious, but recovered during the descent. The pressure of the atmosphere at such a height is 260 millimetres. The greatest mountain heights yet at- tained are 23,100 feet (Aconcagua, in the Southern Andes), reached by Fitzgerald, and 23,400 feet (Trisul, in the Garhwal Himalayas), reached by Dr. Longstaff and his companions. The pressure at this height was 320 millimetres. From these facts it is clear that mountaineers have just about reached the limit ; but since they have not as yet mounted to a height at which the barometric pressure is less than 300 millimetres, it is possible that slightly higher mountains are still waiting to be conquered. At 23,000 feet the oxygen contained in arterial blood does not exceed 10 volumes per cent. (cf. p. 190). It is therefore about half the normal amount. Hence the breath- lessness and sense of feebleness experienced by climbers. The least exertion leads to the consumption of all the circulating oxygen. But since the effects of want of oxygen are felt at altitudes much lower than those to which reference has been made, it is clear that the question cannot be regarded as simply ore of physics. The nervous system suffers when an attempt is made to do work with a deficient oxygen-supply. Violent headache and nausea attack most persons long before a level is reached at which the combination of hemoglobin with oxygen ceases to be possible. The occurrence of this “‘ mountain sick- ness’? reminds us that we must not take for granted that the nervous system will continue to do its work right up to the - altitude at which oxy-hemoglobin is dissociated. Still, the figures show that, apart from these nervous symptoms, which disappear after a time, no serious disturbance occurs even though the atmospheric pressure be but little higher than the absolute minimum at which hemoglobin combines with oxygen. The capacity of the blood for rapidly absorbing oxygen in aN THE BODY AT WORK _ | the lungs and readily parting with it to the goatee is nate ; and completely explained by the property which hemoglobin © possesses of forming an unstable compound with this gas. It is quite otherwise with regard to the liberation of carbonic acid. The problems presented by the solution of this gas in blood and its elimination in the lungs are difficult to solve. Less than one-tenth of the volume of carbonic acid which can be extracted from blood by the air-pump is simply in solu- tion. The remainder is in loose chemical combination, the chief agents in holding it being the alkaline carbonates which the plasma contains. With an excess of carbonic acid they form acid carbonates, which give up carbonic acid and again become normal carbonates in the lungs. About one-third of the carbonic acid is, however, held by the blood-corpuscles— partly in virtue of their alkaline carbonates and phosphates, partly in combination with their globulin. The affinity of these several vehicles for carbonic acid is sufficient to enable them to take it from the lymph, and to hold it while the blood is in the veins. When they reach the capillaries of the lungs, they part with their burden of carbonic acid to the air. It is in connec- tion with this renunciation that certain difficulties remain to be explained. The carbonic acid is given up with greater readi- ness than our knowledge of the chemistry of the compounds into which it enters in the blood would lead us to expect. Why does oxygen enter blood as it circulates through the lungs, and carbonic acid leave it? We have referred to the immense surface which the lungs expose to air. If a soap- bubble be filled with a mixture of oxygen, nitrogen, and carbonic acid, and if the oxygen be in smaller proportion, and the carbonic acid be in greater proportion, than in the air of the room, oxygen will enter the bubble, and carbonic acid will leave it, by diffusion. If, instead of filling a bubble with gas, we fill a bladder with water charged with carbonic acid, but destitute of dissolved oxygen, a similar exchange with the gases of the air will take place. It is merely a question of “‘ gaseous tension.” The tension of the gases in the lungs is measured by passing a small tube down the trachea, and along one of the two chief bronchi until it becomes blocked in a bronchus just large enough to admit it. Respiration is carried on under normal conditions in the remainder of the lung ; but in the lobe P _ + RESPIRATION — 180 which the catheter blocks diffusion from stationary air to tidal is no longer allowed. At the same time, since the circulation is not interfered with, the gases in the blood of the occluded lobe of the lung are not in markedly different proportions from those in the air-chambers of other parts. If at the end of a sufficient interval the air of the occluded lobe is drawn off and its gases measured, their tensions can be compared with the tensions of gases in specimens of arterial and of venous blood. If from 10 c.c. of fluid 1 c.c. of gas can be removed by the air-pump, the volume of gas dissolved is 10 per cent. of the volume of the fluid which dissolved it. Commonly this is written “ 10 volumes per cent.” To ascertain experimentally the tension of a particular gas in a particular fluid when dissolved to the amount of 10 volumes per cent. at the ordinary pressure of the atmosphere and at the temperature of the body, it would be necessary to place it in an open vessel in air containing a sufficient admixture of the gas to prevent its escape from the fluid. Suppose that it were found that, when the fluid containing the dissolved gas was placed in air mixed with the same gas to the extent of one-tenth of its volume, the fluid neither gave up gas nor absorbed more gas, the tension of the gas would be equal to one-tenth of an atmosphere. Since the pressure of the atmosphere equals 760 millimetres of mercury, the tension of the dissolved gas would be 76 millimetres. If more gas were added to the air, more would dissolve in the fluid ; if some of the gas were removed from the air, gas would escape from the fluid. Gas passes from the medium in which its tension is high to the medium in which its tension is low. The tension of carbonic acid in tissues, particularly in muscles and glands, is higher than in lymph ; in lymph higher than in blood ; in blood higher than in air. Hence it passes by these several stages from the tissues in which it is formed to the air in the lungs. Much ingenuity has been devoted to perfecting methods for the determination of the tension of carbonic acid in lymph and in venous blood. Frequently results have been obtained which seemed opposed to the doctrine that carbonic acid progresses from one medium to another in accordance with the law of pressures ; but such perplexing results were probably due either to imperfections in method or to the establishment of abnormal physiological conditions during the course of the fant in the tissue-spaces where the exchange occurs. “The “= experimenter in such a case was in error in supposing that the a spetimen of lymph which he examined contained as much carbonic acid as did the lymph in the tissue-spaces from which the blood which he compared with it received its supply of this as. ; We have already given the figures for the composition of the air in the air-chambers of the lungs. The figures commonly accepted as correct for the percentages of the several gases in the blood are, at 0° C. and 760 millimetres of mercury pressure : Oxygen. fag nea Nitrogen, - In 100 vol. of arterial blood .. =. 20 39 1-2 In 100 vol. of venous blood .. 8-12 46 1-2 This table shows the gain in oxygen and the loss in car- bonic acid which results from the passage of blood through the capillaries of the lungs. The aerated blood returned to the heart by the pulmonary veins contains 8 to 12 volumes per cent. more oxygen, and about 7 volumes per cent. less carbonic acid, than the blood which the pulmonary artery carries to the lungs. As to the physics of this exchange, the air in the recesses of the lungs contains about 16-36 per cent. of oxygen, and an amount of carbonic acid variously estimated at from 2-57 per cent. to 3-84 per cent. Of the 760 millimetres of mercury which | the atmosphere holds up in a barometric tube, the oxygen 760 x 16-36 oe. in the alveoli of the lungs supports nee 124-33 milli- metres ; the carbonic acid, at the lower figure quoted (2-57 per cent.), 19-5 millimetres. The tension of gases in arterial blood is ascertained by open- ing an artery into a closed vessel which contains nitrogen mixed with oxygen and carbonic acid at about the tensions which it is computed that they have in the blood. If the amounts of these - gases are exactly right, no exchange occurs between the blood and the mixture of gases. The mean of many observations ~ made in thie way aie various physiologists i is, for oxygen in the blood 72-2 millimetres mercury pressure, for carbonic acid 5 ai 20-5 millimetres mercury pressure. At a glance it is seen that, since the tension of oxygen in the blood never exceeds 72 milli- _ metres, whereas its tension in pulmonary air never falls beneath 124 millimetres, there is no difficulty in accounting for its passage from air to blood. The position is somewhat otherwise __ with regard to carbonic acid. Aeration continues in the lungs until the tension of this gas in the blood returning to the heart does not exceed 20-5 millimetres ; whereas the tension in pul- monary air, even accepting the lowest figure obtained by ex- perimental means, is as high as 19-5 millimetres. This leaves a very small margin of pressure to account for the escape—and it is undoubtedly a rapid escape—of carbonic acid from blood as it circulates through the lungs. As was said regarding the fixation of carbonic acid in the blood, it is somewhat doubtful whether the problem has been completely solved. The carbonic acid exhaled contains all the carbon of the digestible food, with the exception of a comparatively small quantity given off in urea. It amounts to about 900 grammes per diem. How are we to determine the quantity of air which an individual requires ? We can but make the general statement that it must be sufficient to dilute the carbonic acid exhaled to an extent which precludes poisoning. It is impossible to fix a limit. Breathing becomes embarrassed, and frontal headache and other symptoms make themselves felt when 10 per cent. of pure carbonic acid is mixed with air. Even in so large a pro- _ portion as this, carbonic acid is not fatal to life. Yet an atmo- sphere in which there is present a hundredth part of this amount of carbonic acid, produced by respiration, is extremely injurious to health under the ordinary conditions in which people live. It may be asserted, therefore, that under ordinary conditions 0-1 per cent. is the extreme limit for wholesome living. But again we are obliged to add that air contaminated to this extent is not under all circumstances injurious to health. The explorers on the recent Antarctic Expedition were obliged at times to sleep three men in one sleeping-bag, with the aperture of the bag tightly closed. The atmosphere must have been heavily laden with carbonic acid. Dr. Wilson assures us that it was OP eater Manne age ——— 7 sah ate ee UE gd tne a ca att rae. impossible to keep a pipe 6 AGES inside the ie Not that any By man so placed would desire, one would imagine, to add the | combustion-products of tobacco to those given off from the lungs! The survival of the explorers proves that it is im- possible to fix a limit of safety even for the carbonic acid in air vitiated by respiration. It is, however, a matter of common observation that air which is moist and warm, owing to respira- tion, and tainted with the odours of humanity, is extremely prejudicial to those who live in it. Such an atmosphere is a favourable medium for the conveyance of germs, whether of the common cold or of a more virulent type. At one time it was supposed that the volatile emanations which can be condensed, along with water, by hanging a vessel of ice to the ceiling of a crowded room, were actively poisonous ; but this statement has not been confirmed by recent research. It is unnecessary to call any such evidence in support of the thesis that human beings thrive better in fresh air than in foul. The admirable results achieved by the “ fresh-air cure ” show that there is no degree of vitiation which can be pronounced innocuous. Never- theless, public opinion demands that sanitarians should give some figure as a guide. Commonly they fix the maximum of carbonic acid compatible with health at 0-06 per cent., the quantity of carbonic acid being taken as the measure of all impurities present. An adult exhales about 0-6 cubic foot of CO, per hour. Fresh air already contains about 0-04 per cent. If, therefore, the percentage is not to rise higher than 0-06 per cent., each adult must be supplied with 3,000 cubic feet of air per hour. With good ventilation air may be changed four times an hour, and therefore 800 cubic feet is regarded as sufficient space for each occupant of aroom. The figure may pass. It is a reasonable basis from which to calculate the packing capacity of a dormitory. So long as a man has 800 cubic feet of air to himself, he may safely feel that he has room to stretch his lungs. Dwelling on this figure may make him feel uncomfortable when he finds himself in a railway carriage, seated five on a side, with the windows closed. In the theatre or in church he may doubt whether he has all the fresh air to which his humanity entitles him. But, as a philosopher rather than as a physiologist, he reflects that, whether on the Antarctic icecap in a sleeping-bag or standing on a summit in —a0, 193 % the Alps, he takes all that he can get, for fresh air is one of the _ few good things of which one can never have enough. Tissue Respiration.—A frog will live for seventeen hours in an atmosphere of nitrogen. Under these circumstances it is clearly impossible for it to take up oxygen, yet for several hours it gives off as much carbonic acid as it would do if it were living in air. Such an observation as this proves that oxidation does not occur in the lungs, but deeper in the body. At one time the blood was regarded as the seat of oxidation ; the products formed by the splitting up of proteins in the tissues were supposed to be passed into the blood, where they came in contact with the oxygen carried by hemoglobin. A certain amount of oxidation does take place in the blood, as in all other tissues, for blood is a living tissue and needs to respire. But the oxidation which occurs in the blood is small in amount as compared with that in the organs which the vessels traverse. Muscle and other tissues detached from the body and free from blood give off carbonic acid. It is possible to wash the blood out of the vessels of a frog and to replace it with a solution of salt. In an atmo- sphere of oxygen such a “ saline frog’”’ lives for a day or two, taking in the same quantity of oxygen and giving off the same quantity of carbonic acid as a normal frog. The oxygen is chiefly absorbed through the skin, the carbonic acid discharged from the lung. This experiment shows that blood is not essential for oxidation. Oxidations do not occur in the salt solution with which blood is replaced. Taking all the evidence together, it seems to be safe to conclude that the tissues absorb the oxygen which the oxyhemoglobin brings into their neigh- bourhood, and that they have some capacity of storing it. A piece of detached muscle which gives off carbonic acid in an atmosphere of nitrogen would appear to be holding a store of oxygen, much as hemoglobin holds it. The proof is not quite so definite as might be desired ; but we are probably justified in holding the belief that the main part of the respiratory exchange occurs in the tissues. Lymph dissolves oxygen which it obtains from the blood. The tissues take it from lymph. Tissues set free carbonic acid which lymph dissolves. Its tension being higher than in blood, carbonic acid diffuses from lymph, through the walls of the capillary vessels, into blood, from which it passes into the air in the lungs. 13 CHAPTER VIII EXCRETION Many things enter into the alimentary canal. If an analysis were made of a day’s food and drink, from the cup of tea on waking to the cocoa or other potion which is regarded as a necessary preliminary to settling for the night, it would be found that a great variety of substances were included in the food or taken as adjuvants to food. All these things, differing widely in chemical constitution, must leave the body. Some are not digested. They do not, properly speaking, enter into the diet. Such are the cellulose of vegetables, especially skins, husks, woody fibres ; elastic fibres of meat ; horny substances, etc. The quantity varies greatly, according to the nature of the diet. About 2 ounces (weighed dry) is the average. With this indigestible refuse is included undigested food, if the diet be excessive, and a variety of substances secreted by the liver, such as cholesterin and bile-pigment, some residues of the secretions of the alimentary canal, and products of bacteric fermentations. All food which is digested and absorbed is oxidized. It leaves the body by the lungs, the kidneys, or the skin. Foods, as already stated, are classified as proteins, carbohydrates, and fats. The chief excreta are carbonic acid, water, and urea. Carbonic acid makes its exit from the lungs ; water from the lungs, the kidneys, and the skin ; urea from the kidneys. The three great groups of foods and the three great groups of excreta overshadow in amount all the other sub- stances which pass through the system. A balance-sheet in which proteins, carbohydrates, and fats appear on one side, carbonic acid, water, and urea on the other, is substantially correct. The energy which is set free by burning in a calori- meter the items entered on the debit side, after deducting that yielded by burning the urea (carbonic acid and water are in- 194 * “4 ri "EXCRETION: 195 & - capable of - further oxidation), gives a ae s income. Other constituents of the diet are so small in quantity as to be negli- gible in making up the body’s accounts. The chemical changes which they undergo add practically nothing to its capacity for work. Yet some of them are essential to the maintenance of health. Of such are common salt (sodic chloride), alkaline and earthy carbonates, sulphur, phosphorus, etc. These things, together with some products of action of the bacteria in the-~- alimentary canal, the final stage of hemoglobin, imperfectly oxidized nitrogenous substances, and other soluble substances which enter with, or are formed from the food, are removed by the kidneys. We speak of the elimination of waste products, as excretion. Not that there is any physiological distinction between excretion and secretion. Both terms refer to the selection or production and the discharge of materials by cells. If the product discharged has a useful function to perform—if it be a digestive ferment, for example—it is said to be secreted. If it is of no further use to the economy, we say that it is ex- creted—got rid of. In some cases either term is equally appro- priate. The sebum prepared by the sebaceous glands is useful as a lubricant of the skin. It is thrown off. We may speak of the glands as either secreting or as excreting this fatty sub- stance. The Kidney.—From worms upwards, all animals possess organs for the removal of waste products in solution. This statement might, indeed, be widened so as to include animals even lower than worms. All animals which have a ccelomic cavity—a space between the alimentary canal and the body- -wall—have organs for the removal of soluble waste. The seg- mental organs of worms are obviously the same organs as the kidneys of mammals ; the latter are distinguished from their prototypes by greater concentration of structure and specializa- tion of function. The kidney is the oldest of organs, if its antiquity be estimated as the length of time during which it has had a form practically identical with that which it now pre- sents. The lungs are of late appearance in the animal scale. Alimentary canal, heart, brain, have passed through many transformations. The kidney assumed its permanent form very far back in the history of the animal kingdom. The most primitive animal which has a digestive cavity, and 13—2 196 | THE BODY AT WORK vessels in which the products of digestion circulate, needs an — organ which provides for the overflow from the body-fluids of all substances which are injurious or effete. The kidney is an aggregation of long urinary tubules. The head of each tubule is dilated into a globular capsule, into which a tuft of bloodvessels depends. This is the sink into which the waste-water of the blood drips. The long urinary tubules are lined with cells well qualified by form and con- stitution to search the blood in the capillaries which border them, for substances which, not being easily diffusible, have to be forcibly dragged from it and added to the water trickling down the pipe which connects the rain-water head with the sewer. The hydrostatic conditions of this apparatus— the provision for greater or less flow of blood through the tufts (glomeruli) which hang in the capsules, and for longer or shorter exposure of the blood to the purifying activity of the epithelium of the renal tubules—will be described after a very brief account has been given of the structure of the organ. The outer border of the kidney is convex, its inner border concave. The concavity is termed the “hilus.”” The central depression of the hilus is embraced by the expanded end of the ureter—the tube which carries the secretion of the kidney to the bladder. The renal artery and the renal nerves enter, and the renal vein leaves, the kidney at the hilus. If a kidney be split longitudinally, it will be noticed that its outer part, the cortex, is darker in colour than its inner part, the medulla (Fig. 9). The glomeruli already referred to occur in the cortex. The medulla is occupied by radiating tubules, collected into groups. Those of each group converge towards a common duct. From twelve to eighteen ducts open into the expanded end of the ureter, each at the apex of a pyramid. If the section of the kidney be examined with a lens, it will be seen that narrow rays from the medulla extend into the cortex. The cortex is therefore made up of interdigitating pyramids of dark substance, consisting of glomeruli and the contorted tubules, about to be described, and of lighter sub- stance, consisting of straight tubules continuous with those of the medulla. The urinary tubules are the separate pieces of apparatus of which the kidney consists. The problems connected with nS og ) CRETION pry * 197 subule are therefore the problems of the kidney as a Fia. 9.—THE UPPER END OF THE LEFT KIDNEY, VERTICALLY DIVIDED, AND MAGNIFIED. It is invested by a capsule with which, at the hilus, the dilated end of the ureter blends. A portion of a papilla (the end of a pyramid) is shown projecting into one of the calices into which the ureter dilates. The peripheral portion of the kidney containing glomeruli and contorted tubes is termed its cortex, the central portion medulla. At A is shown a single urinary tubule. Commencing at the third glomerulus, it winds in the cortex, descends into the medulla, turns in a loop of Henle, again winds in the cortex, and ends in a collecting tube, which joins a duct. The arrangement of the bloodvessels is shown at B. A straight artery and a straight vein lie side by side. The artery gives branches to the glomeruli. The venules from the glomeruli again divide into capillaries, which supply the contorted tubes and loops of Henle. The ducts are supplied by long arterial capillaries. C shows the structure (magnified) of a glomerular tuft of capillary vessels, invested by a capsule which closes into a contorted tube, ct ; dH, a descending limb; aH, an ascending limb of a loop of Henle; d, a duct. tion of any one of them applies to all. Each begins as a capsule containing a glomerulus. The wall of the bulb—which BEE ce pe We ted hake ence et ee PS ine Sete |e at be nD a ae a on ESS Ne eg ee Ee NOSES ear es Ue ee oe os aay eae Rey Py ee ay Sa es ‘re s Es be dar ees 5 ae wry sob eps LS Pye oe ee 198 ‘THE BODY AT WORK is merely a thin basement membrane covered by epithelial scales—is involuted by the tuft of bloodvessels. The vessels do not penetrate its capsule. Between the tessellated epi- thelium which covers the tuft and the similar epithelium which lines the capsule there is a space communicating by a narrow aperture with the next portion of the tubule—termed its “contorted ”’ part, because it is twisted about like a tangled thread in the cortex of the kidney. The contorted tubule is of relatively large calibre. The cells which line it are irregular in form and indistinct in outline. The basal half of each cell, between its nucleus and the basement membrane, is vertically striated, or ‘‘ rodded,”’ as it is usually termed. Such an arrangement of the protoplasm of a cell is commonly associated with a habit of absorbing fluid. It would seem to indicate in this case that the cells take water and various sub- stances dissolved in water from the direction of the basement membrane. After a time the contorted portion of the tubule, although still sinuous, becomes more nearly straight—the “spiral portion ’’—and assumes a radial direction. In the zone between the cortex and the medulla, the spiral portion tapers into an exceedingly slender tubule which, after running some distance in the direction of the hilus, turns back again towards the cortex, making a loop, known as the “loop of Henle.” The ascending limb of this loop is of larger calibre than the descending limb. The descending limb is lined by flattened epithelium, each cell so thin that (in microscopic sections as ordinarily prepared) its nucleus bulges into the lumen of the tube. The cells of the ascending limb are more nearly cubical in form. On reaching the cortex, the tubule again becomes contorted. The second contorted portion narrows into a “collecting portion,” which joins a ductule. The ductules unite together, until at last a single duct is formed which opens at the apex of a pyramid. The cells of the duc- tules are cubical or columnar. Their cell-substance is clear, whereas that of the cells lining other parts of the tubule is cloudy in appearance. Such a tubule, viewed as a hydrostatic mechanism, presents three portions, evidently fitted for different functions : (1) The glomerulus is an apparatus which allows of the rapid exuda- tion of water from blood. (2) The contorted portions of the 4 tubule present the appearance of a secreting siechiniam. The large soft, cloudy cells which line them are eminently fitted to take from the blood, or rather from the lymph which fills the tissue-spaces which intervene between the walls of the capillary bloodvessels and tubules, the various substances which they excrete. (3) The loop of Henle is a remarkable piece of apparatus, the purpose of which has been a subject of much controversy. Looking at it from the point of view of hydro- statics, it seems safe to conclude, from its extremely narrow bore, that it raises the pressure of the fluid in the glomerulus and first contorted portion; but it may have other functions also. A consideration of the arrangement of the bloodvessels of _ the kidney bears out the conclusion that the secreting apparatus is divisible into at least two separate portions, possibly into three. The glomeruli are supplied by short and relatively wide arterioles. Each arteriole breaks up, as soon as it enters the capsule, into a bunch of capillary vessels, which, in the same abrupt manner, reunite to form a venule. On leaving the capsule, this little vein behaves in a fashion for which the only parallel is to be found in the portal system of the liver. Instead of uniting with a larger vein, it again breaks up into capillary vessels, which supply the contorted tubules and loops of Henle. The medulla of the kidney is supplied by long arterial capillaries of the usual type. The short arterioles of the glomeruli are controlled by nerves which, constricting them, or allowing them to dilate—possibly by actively causing them to dilate—rapidly diminish or increase the amount of blood passing through their tufts of capillary vessels. Here, _ therefore, is a mechanism by which the glomeruli can be sud- denly flushed with blood—a condition favourable to exudation into the urinary tubules. The interposition of a second set of capillaries prevents this sudden flushing from unduly disturb- ing the pressure in the vascular system as a whole. In the renal-portal capillaries of the kidney the blood-pressure is fairly constant and, presumably, low. The use of the term *‘renal-portal ”’ is justifiable, not only on the ground that the vessels of the kidney behave like those of the portal system of the liver, but also owing to the very significant fact that in fishes and amphibia the kidney actually has a double blood- supply. In such an animal as the frog the glomeruli are supplied with arterial, the tubules ih venous, ‘Tplood: “the glomeruli receive branches from the renal artery, the tubules © zi from a portal system derived from veins of the abdomen and hind-legs. Sir William Bowman, who in 1842 gave the first detailed "3 "a description of the microscopic structure of the kidney, con- cluded that, whereas “ the tubes and their plexus of capillaries are probably the parts concerned in the secretion of that portion of the urine to which its characteristic properties are due (the urea, lithic acid, etc.), the Malpighian bodies [7.e., the glomeruli] may be an apparatus destined to separate ros the blood the watery portion.” All physiologists are in accord in regarding the glomeruli as the principal seat of exudation. There is great diversity of view as to the function of the tubules. In 1844 Ludwig advanced the opinion that all the constituents of the urine pass through the glomeruli in a large excess of water, and that in the course of the tubules this excess of water is re- absorbed. This theory was based, among other considerations, upon the extreme thinness of the epithelium which covers the glomerular tufts ; he judged that water would filter through it very readily. A large amount of experimental work has been directed to the solution of these two problems—viz., (1) Do urea and other similar substances pass through the glomeruli ? (2) Is water returned from the tubules to the venous system ? Our views as to the functions of the kidney as a whole will not be greatly influenced by the answers that may eventually be given to these questions ; yet their discussion is of very great interest, owing to the nature of the evidence which may be marshalled on either side. There is, perhaps, no other organ in the body the problems with regard to which seem to be so nearly plain questions of hydrostatics. It is easy to make a model of a urinary tubule and its blood-supply. If such a model were shown to a sanitary engineer, and he were asked to explain the working of the drainage system of the body, and especially to answer the two questions which we have propounded, he would say that there could be no doubt as to the part of it through which most water enters the tube, the glomerulus. He could give no opinion as to whether urea, uric acid, and other substances of a like A Brey i =i As) ,, ¥ cs EXCRETION 201 nature, accompany the water until he had tried the experiment of separating blood from water containing the inorganic salts of urine by a permeable membrane—the blood being at such a pressure as the physiologist told him he might expect it to have in renal arterioles, the water at such a pressure as he might expect it to have at the upper end of a urinary tubule. He would find that urea, and still more uric acid, is very reluctant to pass through the membrane. Again, when asked whether water, in which urea and other things were dissolved, would leave the tubule—say from the loop of Henle—to pass back into the blood, he would repeat his experiment with a mem- brane. This time he would allow the urine and the blood to be at the same pressure (or, possibly, would assign a higher pressure to the former), and he would dilute the urine to make the conditions agree with those which Ludwig supposed to exist ; but his experiment would prove to him that, unless the urine were very dilute indeed, water would still tend to pass into it from the blood, and not vice versa. And here it may be remarked that the results of these experiments might have been predicted by calculation. When Ludwig advanced his theory, osmosis was a mysterious phenomenon. Its laws have since been accurately ascertained. Given the molecular weights of bodies in solution and their degree of concentra- tion, the direction in which they will pass through a mem- brane can be predicted. The force with which water will tend to pass from one solution to another can be calculated. Urine as secreted contains far more urea, sodic chloride, and other salts than blood. It has a much higher degree of con- - centration. The concentration of blood is 0:55 ; that of urine, 1-85. Water passes from a less concentrated to a more con- centrated solution, not vice versa. As a solution of a problem in hydrostatics Ludwig’s hypothesis is untenable. Osmosis.—Cells of all kinds, both vegetable and animal, are limited, or surrounded by a layer of cell-substance which is firmer than, and probably different in constitution from, the substance in the interior of the cell. This outer layer is a living membrane. The nutrition and growth of the cell are dependent upon the capacity of its limiting membrane for regulating the ingress and egress of water and of substances dissolved in water. ‘The phenomena of osmosis—that is to vw Moet A Bey A eps meee tisk mes eT, as a Rs = eer res: ee =a! ed tes =* 202 = THE BODY AT WORK say, of the passage of water and of solutions through mem- hs branes—are of such high importance in relation to the life of the tissues that it may be permissible to make a further digres- sion for the purpose of describing them (cf. pp. 40, 128). A very simple apparatus will suffice to exhibit a phenomenon which will give an idea of the meaning of osmosis. If the top of a glass funnel, covered with a piece of bladder, so fastened to its edge as to make it water-tight, be fixed in an inverted position in a glass vessel, the glass vessel filled with water, and the funnel filled to the same level with a solution of sugar, it will soon be evident that water is passing through the membrane into the funnel. The level of the sugar-solution will rise in the tube of the funnel. If, instead of water outside the funnel and sugar-solution inside it, a strong solution of sugar be placed in the funnel and a weaker solution outside it, water will leave the weaker for the stronger solution, and sugar the stronger solu- tion for the weaker. If some of the solution in the funnel be removed from time to time so that the pressure in it is kept down to the same level as that outside it, water will continue to enter through the membrane and sugar to leave the contents of the funnel until the concentration of sugar is the same on the two sides. The fluids will then be of identical composition, and therefore isosmotic. In the further con- sideration of the phenomena of osmosis, a distinction must be made between permeable and hemipermeable membranes. Suppose in the first instance that a permeable membrane is used. Let it be so placed as to separate two watery solutions of different constitution, yet of the same osmotic pressure. By their being of the same osmotic pressure is meant that they are of the same molecular concentration. The liquid A contains certain salts in solution ; but the liquid B may con- tain the same salts in quite different proportions. It so happens, however, that the salts are so balanced that the total tension of the salts in A is equal to the total tension of the salts in B. At first there may be some change in level in the two liquids, owing to differences in rates of diffusion through the mem- brane of the various salts which they contain ; but after a time the levels of the two liquids will be the same. To outward appearance, nothing will have happened. Nevertheless, if the experiment has been continued for a sufficient length of time, ; wil ek found that ‘seo Woihes have occurred in shi con- | F ‘stitution of the two liquids. At the commencement, g aoe their total tensions were equal, the proportions in which the various salts were distributed in A, and therefore their partial tensions, were very different to their proportions and partial tensions in B. At the end of the experiment each of the several salts is equally divided between A and B, supposing the volume of A to equal that of B. This experiment shows that the molecules of substances in solu- tion are free to move. They behave like gases. Gases diffuse through a membrane until their partial tensions are the same in the two spaces which the membrane separates. The ether in which physicists picture gases as dissolved offers no re- sistance to the migration of their molecules ; neither does the solvent—water, for example—prevent the movement of salts which are distributed through it. One other illustration of the phenomena of osmosis will suffice to give an idea of the laws by which they are governed. In the case just cited the membrane was permeable to all the salts in solution. When the phenomena of osmosis were first investigated, a distinction was drawn between substances which will pass through membranes—crystalloids—and sub- stances which cannot pass—colloids. We have already had occasion to note that, whereas albumin is a colloid which does not diffuse, its hydrate, peptone, is a crystalloid which does. The term “ crystalloid’ indicates that substances which can be crystallized are diffusible. Substances which are diffusible are therefore allied to those which crystallize. The nature of the membrane used to test diffusibility was not at first taken into account. Now a distinction is drawn between membranes which are permeable to all diffusible substances, and membranes which are permeable to the solvent, but im- permeable to the substances which it dissolves. The latter are termed “ hemipermeable.” Imagine now that water is separated from a solution of sugar by a membrane which stops sugar, but is permeable to water. Water will pass through the membrane into the solution of sugar. The level of the solution will rise. Pressure will be needed, and a very con- siderable pressure, to prevent its rising—to prevent endos- mosis, that is to say. The force needed to resist osmosis is 204 THE BODY AT WORK directly proportional to the degree of concentration of the solution. If the solution contain 1 per cent. of sugar, a pressure of 500 millimetres of mercury is needed ; if it contain 2 per cent., a pressure of 1,000 millimetres; if 6 per cent., of 3,000 millimetres. In the next experiment separate two solutions, A and B, by a hemipermeable membrane. Let A contain one salt only—X ; let B contain several salts—X, Y, Z. Water will pass from A to B, or vice versa, unless the osmotic pressure of the salts which the solutions contain is the same, The osmotic pressure will be found to be the same if the total number of molecules dissolved in A equals the total number of molecules dissolved in B. If in A there be N molecules of X (per unit volume), and if in B there be nX, n’Y, n”Z, the osmotic pressure will be the same providedn+n’+n"=N. This, it will be seen, is a very different matter from equality of percentage composition. Some mole- cules are light; others are heavy. The percentage weight of X+Y+Z in B may be very different from the percentage weight of X in A. To estimate the osmotic pressure of a mixed solution, it is not sufficient to add together the per- centages of the various salts which it contains. ‘‘ Concentra- tion,”’ in the sense in which it was used in regard to blood and urine, refers to the number of molecules of dissolved substances in a given volume, not to their weight. It would be undesirable to attempt in this place to enter upon the theory of osmosis. Enough has been said to suggest to the reader that he should, when endeavouring to apply its laws to the explanation of physiological phenomena, bear the following facts in mind: Some membranes are permeable to water and to the crystalloids which it dissolves; others, although permeable to water, are impermeable to substances in solution. Some substances are diffusible through per- meable membranes ; others are not. Osmosis of water occurs from the solution of lower to the solution of higher concentra- tion. Diffusion of crystalloids is their escape, owing to their own molecular movements, from a situation in which they are denser to a situation in which they are less dense. It must be added, however, that various circumstances prevent the re- duction of the laws of osmosis to simple terms—the tendency of salts to dissociate when in solution, their bases and acids acting ale ey oan! ge OE a Oe, ON Poet pe ee ee +) 4 "3 ee ee te ater ene em t + Pe fs oc¥ eae gee n EXCRETION 205 as independent ‘‘ions,” is an example of the complications which produce apparent departures from these laws. It must further be added, and with emphasis, that, important though it be that anyone who attempts to explain the interchanges which occur between the various fluids of the body should be conversant with the laws of osmosis, it is impracticable, and in some cases misleading, to rigidly apply them. Living mem- branes and dead membranes do not necessarily control diffusion in the same manner. Still. less do the laws which govern diffusion through dead membranes hold good, without qualifica- tion, to living cells. To return to the sanitary engineer whose opinion we asked regarding the mode of working of the drainage system of the kidney. Probably he would deny that the problems came within his province. “‘ They are not physical, but vital,’ he would say. “I know nothing about the vital action of the cells which line the tubule.” Objection may be taken to the _ form of expression, albeit he was fully justified in declining to discuss the question any further. He does not know enough about the internal structure of a cell to be able to predict the phenomena of osmosis which will occur within it. No one can say what capacity living cells may have of taking substances from the blood, returning some of them, and excreting others. This unknown capacity leads to results which, when they do not appear to be in accordance with the laws of physics, are commonly termed “ vital.” The term is a stumbling- block which has tripped up generations of physiologists. The expressions ‘‘ vital action’ and “ physical phenomena ”’ have been used as if they were antithetical, whereas all vital actions are physical phenomena. “Vital’’ in this sense connotes ‘‘as yet unknown.” Yet, in truth, there is abundant excuse for the use of a term which covers ignorance, so long as its con- notation is not extended until it assumes a positive, anti- physical sense. “Physical”? and “vital” are expressions which point a contrast constantly present to a physiologist’s mind. He knows perfectly well that the passage of water and - salts through a membrane, and their passage into and out of a living cell, are equally phenomena of osmosis. But the former process he can test and measure in his laboratory ; the latter he can but observe in much obscurity in the living body. He Shs areas Pen Be GTA Cwhiel oe TM steel ae Nem Ta 3.) hey Oa a eS : ne ee 206 THE BODY AT WORK — cannot make a model of a living cell. In the case of the salivary gland, as we have already seen, living cells take water from lymph, and discharge it as saliva in apparent opposition to osmotic force. They reverse the direction of the flow which would occur were lymph and saliva separated by a membrane. But a cell is not a membrane. It is an extremely complicated structure with an elaborate architecture of its own. As well might we compare the distribution of water by a County Council water-cart and its passage through a brewery. Accord- ing to all the laws of hydrostatics, the water which flows into a brewery should leave it through its drains. Its exit in barrels on drays is antiphysical. When the physiologist can explore the living cell, he will discover that the imbibition and extrusion of water, the selection, retention, and discharge of salts, are phenomena as strictly physical as their passage through a dialyser in his laboratory. In the meantime he can but con- template the cell with a certain degree of awe. His best devised model of a urinary tubule may lead him into error, for the simple reason that he cannot line it with living cells. A living cell has a power which upsets all calculations, falsifies all experimental findings. Its protoplasm can isolate and place out of action any of the substances which enter it. If observa- tions eventually prove to us that water passes from the urinary tubules into the blood, “‘ in the face of osmotic force,”’ we shall be constrained to explain this antiphysical phenomenon as due to the action of living cells. The cells, we shall say, take up fluid from the urinary tubules, fix its urea and other salts in their protoplasm, discharge its water into the venous blood, return the urea and other salts to the urine. Given this property of protoplasm, such a process is strictly in accordance with physical laws. Enough has been said regarding the theory, or want of theory, of the action of the kidney. Turning now to matters of obser- vation, it can easily be shown that the epithelium of the tubules has the power of excreting into the urine highly complex materials which diffuse with difficulty. If a substance soluble in blood, but insoluble in urine, an alkaline salt of indigo, for — example, be injected into the vascular system, it is rapidly excreted by the kidney. The indigo is precipitated even before mre we ; A aes ad EXCRETION 207 r it comes in contact with the acid urine. If the animal be killed a short time after the administration of the indigo, the con- _ torted portions of its tubules and the ascending limbs of the loops of Henle are strongly coloured blue. An ammoniacal solution of carmine may be used for a similar experiment ; but the results are not nearly so sharply limited to the large-celled portions of the tubules. Even the glomerulus is coloured red, a fact which has been interpreted as showing that, although the greater part of the carmine is excreted into the tubules, some of it accompanies the water which exudes from the blood through the glomerular tufts. The practical identity in structure of the kidney in birds and reptiles and mammals would seem to have an important bearing on this controversy. The urinary excretion of birds consists almost exclusively of uric acid. As seen under the microscope, it is a semisolid white deposit, made up of crystals, supposing no special precautions have been taken to obtain it fresh. The water, pigment, and salts which are essential elements of the excretion of mammals are practically absent. Yet the kidney of a bird presents the same arrangement of glomeruli and tubules as the kidney of a mammal, although the glomeruli are relatively smaller. Uric acid diffuses with great difficulty. If it is, so to speak, washed through the glomeruli, and the water which dissolved it reabsorbed by the tubules, an enor- mous quantity of water must pass through the kidney in order that it may carry the uric acid in its stream. If uric acid be excreted by the epithelium of the tubules, it is difficult to account for the presence of glomeruli, since no water leaves the _kidney. Crystals of uric acid are to be seen in a section of the kidney, not only in the cells of the tubules, but also in the glomeruli; but it may well be that in both situations crystalliza- tion has been induced during the preparation of the section. It jars an histologist’s conception of the constitution of a secreting cell to contemplate the formation within its network of proto- plasm, and the extrusion from it, of sharp-angled crystals. As a matter of fact, it is not in its crystalline form that uric acid is excreted by birds, but as quadri-urates—i.e., salts containing only one-fourth of their ‘normal’ complement of base ; crystalline spheres or amorphous deposit, not angular crystals. These quadri-urates decompose very quickly, setting free A tS ee ee eee ee 208 : THE BODY AT WORK — crystals of uric acid. It must be confessed that,in whatever way one attempts to account for the excretion of uric acid by birds, the similarity of structure of their kidneys and those of mammals is difficult to reconcile with the wide difference in consistency and in chemical composition of the excrement. — Reflecting upon all the evidence bearing upon the mechanism of the mammalian kidney, the majority of physiologists come — to the following conclusions: The greatest outflow of water occurs in the glomeruli. The water is accompanied by salts, including a small quantity of urea. The contorted and spiral portions of the tubule and the ascending limbs of Henle’s loops add to the urine the remainder of the urea, together with various bodies still less readily diffusible. It may be that the chief function of the loops of Henle is to oppose resistance to the passage of fluids, thus heading up the secretion, and favouring the osmosis of water into it from the blood of the glomerular capillaries. It is possible that the calibre of the slender descending limbs is influenced by external pressure, their partial occlusion being increased, and the pressure in them raised, when the organ is very active and its intermediate zone turgid with blood. Various drugs influence the secretion of the kidney. In some cases their action seems to be mainly hydrostatic. They change the rate of flow by altering blood-pressure. Digitalis in- creases the force of the heart. The heart beating more strongly, blood-pressure rises. Higher blood-pressure is accompanied by a more copious secretion. This action of digitalis is far more marked when the heart is out of order than when it is healthy. In heart-disease the blood-pressure is unduly low, and the tissues become water-logged in consequence. When the _blood- pressure is restored and a brisker capillary circulation estab- lished, water and waste-products, which have accumulated in lymph, pass, as they ought to do, into the veins. Carried into the general circulation, they overflow from the kidney. It is a little difficult to realize the abundance of the body- fluids. From one-quarter to one-third of the whole body- weight is due to lymph, using this term in its most general sense. The waste-products of tissues collect in the lymph. The blood circulating through capillary vessels which traverse lymph-spaces takes up water and waste-products. Its just - EXCRETION 209 a peuapoatbion is maintained by the eliminating activity of the _ kidneys. Even in the diuretic action of digitalis we see indications of something more than an alteration of the hydrostatics of the blood-supply of the kidney. The brisker circulation carries waste-products to the liver ; the liver transforms nitrogenous refuse into urea; urea stimulates the renal epithelium. It would be a mistake to lay too much stress upon the direct effect of the drug upon the blood-pressure in the kidney. Other illustrations throw the mere hydrostatics of the problem into the background. Adrenalin (extract of suprarenal capsule) causes a severe contraction of the small arteries, which raises the general blood-pressure considerably ; but the increased blood- pressure is not accompanied by diuresis, because the glomerular arterioles share to a full extent, perhaps to a disproportionate extent, in the general constriction. In migraine and certain _other disorders it frequently happens that the blood-pressure in the aorta is unduly high, yet very little fluid enters the renal tubules. If a “saline diuretic,” potassic nitrate, sodic acetate, or some other drug of the same kind, be administered, a copious flow is established, the blood-pressure is relieved, the distressing symptoms disappear. Then, again, certain diuretics, such as ‘ sweet spirits of nitre,” tea, gin, etc., may bring about a flow out of all proportion to the alteration they produce in the hydrostatics of the circulation. The diuretic action of these various drugs is clearly due to increase in permeability of the renal epithelium. And, of all stimulants to secretion, urea, the natural stimulant, is the most effective. If a kidney be removed from the body, a cannula inserted into its artery, and defibrinated blood caused to circulate under pressure through the organ, water may or may not drip from the ureter. On addition of urea to the blood, a copious excretion is set up. In explaining the mode of working of the kidney, as, indeed, in explaining that of every other organ of the body, the mechanical aspects of the problem must be kept in the background. When we are contemplating the plan of construction of the kidney, the hydrostatics of the circulation attract attention; but alterations in hydrostatic conditions are not the initiating cause of a greater or less flow of urine. The chemical condition of the blood circulating through the kidney is the initiating cause, 14 ar THE BODY AT WORK —— When the presence in it of urea demands a more copious — = the hydrostatic conditions are adjusted to this need. In the — case just cited of the isolated kidney, it might be urged that the _ flow caused by urea is a mechanical effect. The cells of the contorted portions of the urinary tubules remove urea from the blood. They secrete it into the tubules. The solution of urea, being headed up towards the glomeruli, owing to the resistance offered to its passage down the tubules by the narrow, descending limbs of Henle’s loops, surrounds the capillary tuft. Urea rapidly attracts water from the blood. &, “are - : : “i =-4e eS AS 2S. | \. > * a ~ aL i‘. rw J. > P : PS as — ? 214 THE BODY AT WORK | we gave as the predominant cause of gout acid fermentations _ in the stomach. It does not, by any means, follow, however, that we were right in correlating imperfect digestion with an excessive formation of uric acid. It may well be that the gouty symptoms to which hampered peptic digestion gives rise are due in larger measure to a disturbance of the composition of the body-fluids which renders them unfit to carry uric acid to ~ the kidneys in such a form, or in such relation to the fluid in which it is dissolved, as will insure its escape into the urinary tubules. The interference with the efficient working of the system caused by accumulation in it of uric acid gives a par- ticular interest to all that is known regarding the nature and origin of this substance. Uric acid has the formula C;H,N,0;. It is a more com- plicated and a more stable body than urea. The deposits of guano in Peru contain uric acid (the excrement of birds) which has remained practically unchanged for years—for centuries, perhaps. Its chemical nature is not completely understood. It can be readily made to yield urea ; and it can be formed by conjugating urea with a nucleus derived from lactic acid (cf. p. 13). Its formula is therefore commonly represented as that of a diureide—a substance containing two urea radicles : (HN—-CO CO , Cz art CO [HN —— C——NH But notwithstanding this inclusion in its molecule of two radicles of urea, it is safe, when one thinks of the contrast between urea and uric acid, to lay stress, in the case of the former, on the binding of nitrogen to hydrogen ; in the case of the latter, on the binding of nitrogen to carbon. Uric acid is soluble with difficulty ; it crystallizes in rhombs. It forms salts, normal and acid. Those which appear in the urine are always acid salts. As a treatment for “stone,” lithia water has long had a reputation which it probably deserves, the acid urate of lithium being the most soluble salt _ EXORETION = ° ~_—_—s2B s as uric acid which the kidney can secrete. When uric acid is in excess in urine, brown crystals of uric acid are deposited as “ oravel”’ soon after it is passed. Even when not in excess, uric acid crystals appear after a sufficient time. In other cases uric acid, when in excess, is thrown down in the form of a cloud of acid urates of sodium and other bases, which renders the urine turbid. These urates are redissolved when the water is warmed. The more fortunate of human beings need never concern themselves with the chemical history of uric acid. It is always present in their body-fluids. It is excreted by the kidney. Its formation is of no greater interest than that of creatinin and other nitrogenous compounds which escape the almost universal reduction to urea. Persons who have a uric acid diathesis are in a very different plight. KEvery scrap of evidence bearing upon its origin is of supreme importance. Unfortunately, the evidence collected as yet is scanty, and its application for remedial purposes impracticable. The only disease in which uric acid is invariably in excess is leucocythemia. This is a condition or habit marked by the presence in the blood of a very great number of white blood-cor- puscles and a paucity of red ones. The connection between this disease and the production of uric acid is made plain by certain experiments in diet. If flesh which contains relatively a large proportion of cell-nuclei is eaten, the uric acid excreted is markedly increased. Sweetbread, especially “‘ neck sweet- bread ’’—1.e., thymus gland—is a mass of comparatively small cells with large nuclei. If thymus gland be substituted for all other meaty foods, the quantity of uric acid appearing in the urine is doubled. A large increase in the quantity of ordinary meat or fish consumed also increases uric acid, because all meat-fibres contain nuclei. If egg-albumin be taken instead of meat, uric acid is not increased. A sudden excess of mus- cular work leads to an increase in uric acid, owing presumably to the unusual activity of the tissues. This used to be very noticeable in the case of young men during the first few days of “training”’ under the old system; but it may have been due to the generous consumption of chops and steaks, rather than to the increase in physical work, and consequent destruc- _ tion of tissue. Nuclei contain nucleo-proteins, which split into =o, Se, Re a De ee oe) proteins and nuclein. Chemically, it is reasonable to attribute to nuclein the parentage of uric acid ; a plausible line of descent _ can be traced. The association of leucocythemia with the production of uric acid is probably due to the destruction of leucocytes which are present in abnormal numbers (cf. p. 53). a Such is the evidence at present in the hands of physiologists. __ Naturally, physicians have endeavoured to turn it to account. | Patients have been recommended to avoid animal foods which contain nucleo-proteins—to take, instead of meat and fish, eggs, milk, cheese, vegetable-albumins. Certain physicians contend that such a diet is followed by the happiest results ; others, equally competent, and perhaps less biassed by ‘“ medical theory ’—the most dangerous of handicaps for anyone who practises an art which must ever remain empirical—are satisfied that equally good results are obtained by excluding from the » diet eggs, milk, and cheese. Physiological discoveries suggest treatment. Modern medicine is in the fullest sense applied physiology. But treatment based upon theory must be con- trolled by unprejudiced observation. It is possible that the gouty diathesis may be held in check in certain cases by the exclusion from the diet of certain kinds of nitrogenous food. The experience of generations has taught us that the injudicious use of such articles of diet as fruit, pastry, sugar, which do not contain nitrogen, is the main factor in inducing an attack of gout ; that imperfect digestion, sluggish circulation, insufficient activity on the part of the kidneys, lead to the accumulation in tissue-spaces of the fons et origo malorum. Even sweet- bread, which with the precision of a chemical experiment increases the production of uric acid by a healthy person, is not necessarily found unwholesome by those who are inclined to gout. It is amongst the most digestible of all meat foods, and easy digestion covers a multitude of metabolic sins. CHAPTER IX THE CIRCULATION THE blood circulates in a closed system of tubes, continuous from the heart back to the heart. The walls of these vessels separate the blood from the tissues. Nowhere, except in the spleen, does it come into contact with any cells other than the lining cells of the vessels in which it flows, and the exception made by the spleen is more apparent than real. The spleen (p. 79) is a kind of sponge invested with a firm capsule. Small arteries discharge their blood into its spaces ; small veins collect it. But the organ is essentially a part of the vascular system. Its spaces take the place of the capillary vessels which connect arteries with veins in other situations. The blood makes a double circuit. From the right heart it passes through the vessels of the lungs. Returning to the left heart, it is driven through the body. Although the heart con- sists of two separate pumps, it makes but asingle organ. Its division into right auricle and ventricle and left auricle and ventricle is but slightly indicated on the surface. In most invertebrate animals the two pumps are distinct. In some the - lung-heart and the body-heart are on opposite aspects of the body. But one must not, when thinking of the morphology of the vertebrate heart, picture it as formed by the juxtaposition of two, originally separate, pumps. Truly, in its very earliest stage of growth, it is represented by two tubes which lie, in the embryo, far apart. But these, before we can speak of the existence of a heart, fuse into a single tube, with four con- _ tractile bulbs in series. As the heart develops, the dilatation at its hinder or venous end and the dilatation at its anterior or arterial end disappear. A partition is formed which divides the two middle bulbs into right and left auricle and right and left ventricle respectively. Immediately after birth the lungs are, 217 vessels of the pew s lungs. auricular septum is perforate. R.Carotid A. L. Carotid A. Artery to right arm I l 4 Artery to left arm 7o Lungs To Lungs \\ oe : Hii} ‘ae oe Hi}}}) From Lungs SS From Lungs From Liver '< To Stomach To Spleen Fig. 10.—THE HEART CUT IN THE PLANE OF ITS LONG AXIS, AND THE VESSELS WHICH OPEN INTO AND OUT OF IT. Chord tendines attach the margins of the auriculo-ventricular valves to musculi papillares which project from the inner aspect of each ventricle. foramen. When the lungs are expanded by the forcible enlargement of the chest-cavity which contains them, their bloodvessels are distended by the same extensile force. Blood is sucked into them from the right side of the heart. » i _ ~~. 7 a “ _ a, Pay 1) : oy i ey } eM aL y Cy", a eee ’ q 5, ty ’ ’ AVS o Ky nm + above the head, and slowly lowering them again, has a remark- able effect in quickening the circulation—increasing the blood- Supply of the brain. Changes of posture, by relieving pressure on subcutaneous veins, removes an impediment to the flow of blood. 3 The second of the factors to which we have referred as adjuvant to the heart’s action is the negative pressure of inspiration. In explaining the effect of this force upon the circulation, the relation of the lungs to the thorax must be taken into account. The box in which the lungs are enclosed is too big for them ; nevertheless, being extensible and elastic, they always fill it. They follow its movements when in inspiration the muscles between the ribs enlarge it, and when in expiration it diminishes again. No air or fluid, save the moisture which lubricates the surface of the pleura, reducing friction, occupies the (potential) space between the lungs and the chest. But the moment the chest is punctured the lungs collapse. Air is sucked into the pleural cavity. The lungs fill the chest only so long as there is neither air nor fluid between it andthem. Lung-tissue is extremely delicate. Hach air-cell is a cup of thin membrane holding together a basket- work of capillary vessels. Solong as the chest-wall is stationary the negative pressure in the pleural cavity has no effect upon these slender tubes. But when the chest expands, the capil- laries are between two minus pressures, the pull of the chest- wall and the resistance offered to the entrance of air into the lungs by the passages through which it has to pass. The calibre of the lung-capillaries is increased, just as it would be increased were they hanging in an air-pump while the piston was drawn out. More blood passes to the left heart through the wider capillaries. Ejected into the aorta, it raises the pressure in the arterial system. A record of the pressure in an artery shows a rhythmic rise for each heart-beat. It shows also a rise with inspiration and a fall with expiration. These larger undulations correspond with the movements of the chest, although they are necessarily somewhat late on respira- tion, for the first effect of the dilatation of the capillaries is to cause them to hold more blood and to deliver less. The first effect of expiration, on the other hand, is to urge on the blood which the dilated vessels contain. In any case a single beat is Yih, ate ieee ert ET GR Pele a, tell ee hy ae a Ce % ~ SK wm oss re =. Me ail i. Se elit en eerie Bete ony Ly SL Eh” San) eee cients eae nee Soe ee ae 5 : > ro Ce => E = eas coe : . . it ee 222 ‘THE BODY AT WORK Shs needed to throw into the aorta the blood which has ‘boa received by the right auricle. The expansion of the chest influences the flow of blood in yet another way. The heart and the great vessels which join and leave it are themselves enclosed within the chest, subject to the negative pressure produced within that cavity by the elasticity of the lungs. The lungs pull upon the pericardium, the membranous covering of the heart. When this pull is increased owing to the forcible expansion of the chest, blood is sucked into the great veins, just as air is sucked into the wind- pipe. The thick-walled aorta, containing blood at high pressure, does not feel the effect of slight variations in the pressure round it. The soft-walled veins are expanded during inspiration to a not inconsiderable degree. What relief a deep yawn gives by hastening a languid circulation! Leaning over an account-book late in the afternoon, every condition is unfavourable to the flow of blood. It accumulates in the legs and in the abdomen. The head is thrown back and the mouth opened wide, while the chest expands in a long deep inspiration. Down on the liver, stomach, and intestines presses the flattened diaphragm, squeezing their blood towards the heart. The negative pressure within the chest sucks this up, and draws down the blood con- tained in the great veins of the neck. The capillaries of the lungs are widened, allowing blood to pass more quickly from the right side to the left side of the heart. ‘The heart responds to the call upon it, throwing all that it receives into the aorta. Only a great effort of the will had kept the pale brain at work ; in the attic it suffers more than organs on the lower storeys from insufficient pressure. For a short time after the yawn it finds itself nourished with an adequate supply of blood. The negative pressure in the thorax is considerable at all times. If a manometer—a U-shaped tube with mercury in its loop—be connected with a cannula passed through the wall of the chest, the difference of level of the mercury in the two limbs of the U is a measure of the force with which the lungs are endeavouring to shrink away from the chest-wall. Even at the end of expiration the mercury in the limb next the chest stands about 6 millimetres higher than the mercury in the outer limb. During a deep inspiration the pressure in the chest falls 30 millimetres below the atmospheric pressure. Hence a eee Meee fg mae PD gs ane oe ; THE CIRCULATION — 993 a E problem is eae of which no completely satisfactory solution has yet been given. How comes it that lymph is not sucked into the pleural cavity ? In health there is no more pleural fluid than just suffices to keep the membrane moist. The endothelial cells which cover the surface of the pleura resist further exudation. Valves in the lymphatic vessels prevent backward flow. Yet in disease, when the pleura is inflamed, lymph pours out quickly, often to be reabsorbed with equal rapidity when the pleurisy subsides. This flow uphill, from a lower to a higher pressure, can be explained only as a pheno- menon due to the “secretory” capacity of endothelium. As an answer to the hydrostatic problem this is hardly satisfactory. The circulation of the blood is the result of the difference between the pressure in the vessels through which it leaves the heart, and that in the vessels through which it is returned. The pressure in the aorta amounts to about 200 millimetres of mercury. In the vene cave it is nil, or, ovals to the aspiration of the thorax, less than nil. The Heart.—Inspection of the liver, the spleen, or ve kidney helps but little to the comprehension of the mechanism of these organs. It is quite otherwise in the case of the heart. Its mechanics being comparatively simple, physiology is concerned with measurements, with the conditions under which it can and cannot work, and with the action upon it of the nervous system and of drugs. The heart of any mammal will suffice for anatomical study. A sheep’s heart is about the same size as that of a man, and exactly similar, save in minute par- ticulars, which do not appreciably affect its mode of working. The heart is a hollow muscle, composed of minute contractile cells. Each cell is a cylinder, about twice as long as it is broad, with an oval nucleus in its centre. There is no impropriety in speaking of the heart as a single muscle. Muscles which we can move at will, “ voluntary muscles,” consist of fibres, each from 1 inch to 2 inches long, and of about the thickness of a piece of thread (Fig. 16). Every fibre is surrounded by a membranous sheath, its sarcolemma, which completely isolates it from the others. Each has its separate nerve-supply. A voluntary muscle- fibre is a cell-complex. The single embryonic cell which grew into the fibre underwent nuclear division until hundreds of | nuclei were formed, but its cell-substance was not divided into territories appertaining to the several finelet) “tn Wee on the other hand, nuclear division has been followed by cell- ee, division ; but minute protoplasmic bridges are left between the __ cells. The whole of the heart-substance is thus in structural _ continuity. The cells are not invested with sarcolemma. As ~ the result of this arrangement, an impulse started in one part of the heart spreads over the whole, with certain limitations as to the directions in which it is able to travel, whereas in voluntary muscle a separate impulse must be delivered to each fibre. The wave of contraction commences in the great veins, the ven cave and pulmonary veins, near their junction with the heart, spreads from cell to cell throughout the auricles, and onwards down the ventricles to the apex of the heart. The ~ substance of the heart has not, however, a homogeneous appear- ance. Its cells are collected into fascicles, which lie in various planes and cross the axis of the heart at various angles. Ina boiled sheep’s heart it is easy to separate one fascicle from another, and to distinguish the sheets into which the fascicles are collected. The four valves of the heart lie in almost the same plane. They are supported by a fibrous plate divided into four rings (Fig. 11). Most of the fascicles are attached to this plate, though some which encircle the auricles are independent of it. With one or with both ends attached to the plate, fascicles loop over the auricles. They run down the ventricles with a twist from right to left. Those on the surface turn in at the apex of the heart, and run up the inner surface of the ventricles. Some of them go to form the free columns which are found on the inner surface of the ventricles, pointing towards the valves—musculi papillares. The fibrous plate which supports the valves cuts off almost all of the muscle which makes the walls of the auricles from that which constitutes the ventricular walls ; but a thin sheet is continued from the inner surface of the auricles down the interventricular septum. Toa considerable extent the walls of the two auricles and of the two ventricles are respectively continuous, insuring synchronous contraction. The arrangement of the fascicles accounts for the changes in form which the heart undergoes when it contracts. Systole commences in the cardiac ends of the venz cavee and pulmonary veins. They empty the last of their blood into the auricles, and close to prevent regurgitation, their mouths not being 1, wi Fr papillares. As soon as ventricular systole has commenced, the wo, ® en the auricles quickly shrink in all dimensions, and F as soon as their contraction is at its height the ventricles _ contract, while the auricles relax. The ventricular wave runs _ from base to apex too rapidly to be followed with the eye, % and ends, owing to the involution of the fascicles, in the musculi auricles relax. After emptying their contents into the aorta and pulmonary artery, the ventricles relax, their contraction giving way first at the apex, and being longest held at the base. Then follows a pause (diastole), during which both auricles and ventricles are flaccid. If the pericardium is open, the heart is Fig. 11.—A SECTION APPROXIMATELY AT RIGHT ANGLES TO THE LONG AXIS OF THE HEART, EXPOSING THE FOUR VALVES WHICH LIE VERY NEARLY IN THE SAME PLANE. The semilunar valve which guards the aperture of the pulmonary artery is the nearest to the breast-bone. seen to become round instead of oval in transverse outline during systole. Itshortens. Its apex twists a little to the right, and projects forward. But if it is within its pericardium the shortening is not accompanied with any displacement of the apex. Instead of the apex mounting, the base descends. The front of the right ventricle, at some little distance from the apex, presses the chest-wall forwards in the fifth intercostal space, about an inch to the inner side of a line falling vertically through the nipple. This pressing forwards is felt as the “impulse of the heart.” The contraction of the heart is not a see-saw of auricles and ventricles. During diastole blood is falling from the veins 15 Po eee SS de gs eS See at ee a en 226 THE BODY AT WORK through the auricles into the ventricles. Ina sense, the auricles are not necessary parts of the double pump. They collect blood while the ventricle is contracting, thus preventing it from heading up in the veins. They save time. Their con- traction completes the filling of the ventricle, so that the instant the ventricular contraction begins blood enters the aorta and pulmonary artery. The Valves.—If ever expressions of admiration were appro- priate in a treatise on the animal body, such preface might be permitted to a description of the cardiac valves. Which means no more than this: Men make pumps. Therefore they are in a position to appreciate the mechanism of the heart. We cannot admire what we do not understand. If we made secret- ing organs or self-contracting springs, glands and muscles would evoke our commendation. We should recognize that — Nature’s apparatus is even better adapted to its work than any that men can make. This is the admission which is forced from us when we study the heart. The apertures connecting auricles and ventricles are ex- tremely wide, allowing the contents of the former to be emptied into the latter almost instantaneously. If we attempted to make a pump fulfilling this condition, we should find that it failed in several respects. In the first place, the rush of fluid from the one chamber into the other would press the flaps of the valves back against the wall of the second chamber. They would cling to the wall, and would not float up quickly into place when the second chamber was squeezed. Let us call the two chambers A and V for brevity’s sake. When V contracted, some of the fluid would be thrown back into A, because, the resistance in that direction being lower than the resistance offered by the column of fluid above the pump (the resistance in the aorta is very high), the contents of V would rush past the margins of the A-V valve. This would happen even though its flaps were not pressed back against the wall. Further, at the height of contraction the membranous valve would bulge backwards into A, making a cup towards V which V could not empty. In the heart these difficulties have been overcome. The tricuspid valve, which separates the right auricle from the right ventricle, has three flaps. The mitral valve, on the left side of the heart, has but two. The flaps are composed of ey 1 RW ee ae ee ee me -OROMNT 1 ney UNIV: 227 tough membrane, but are comparatively thin. The following _ direction for deciding at an autopsy whether or not they were healthy at the time of death was given many years ago by a sur- geon of repute : “‘ You ought to be able to see the dirt under your thumbnail when you place it beneath one of the flaps.”’ Surgery has improved in cleanliness as well as in other ways ; indeed, the possibility of advance has been due to the recognition of the need for transcendental cleanliness. But this is a digres- sion. The margins of the flaps are crenulated.. Threads— chorde tendinee—are attached to them like the stay-ropes of a tent. At their other end these tendons are attached to the musculi papillares already mentioned. The bunch of tendons from each papillary muscle spreads, to be inserted into the contiguous margins of two flaps. We have mentioned some of the difficulties which have been overcome in the construction of the pump. (1) The flaps do not flatten back against the wall of the ventricle during systole of the auricle. It must be re- membered that during diastole of both chambers blood is flowing through the auricle into the ventricle. The latter being partly filled before systole of the auricle commences, the flaps are floated up. This is greatly favoured by the form of the inner wall of the ventricle. It is not flat, but raised in pillars —columnez carnee. The spaces between these pillars cause backwash currents, which lift the flaps and help to bring them into apposition as soon as systole of the ventricle commences. (2) No blood which has entered the ventricle is thrown back into the auricle. The valve “balloons” over the blood in the ventricle before the contraction of the auricle has ceased. The thin margins of its flaps come together with great rapidity. The tendinous cords holding their edges on the ventricular side, they meet, not edge to edge, but folded flap to folded flap. (3) The valve does not bulge into the auricle. On the contrary, at the height of systole it is pulled into the ventricle by the contracting musculi papillares. As the ring to which the valve is attached is diminished in size, by the contraction of the base of the heart, which continues, it will be remembered, until after the apex has begun to relax, the edges of the flaps are folded farther and still farther over by the pull of the musculi papillares, and the blood is squeezed out from between the wall of the ventricle and the indrawn valve. 15—2 Be Gee ee nee Vii 7 OT OWOD mio - 228 THE BODY AT WORK Veen 7 eo =. ‘ The “ semilunar valves,’ which close the apertures into the aorta and pulmonary artery, have each three flaps. The aortic _ semilunar valve, which has the higher pressure to bear, shows its characteristic features in a rather more marked degree thar the other. Each of its three flaps is a half-cup. At the centre of the margin of the half-cup is a small fibrous nodule. The edge of the cup on either side of this is very thin. Fine elastic fibres radiate from the nodule to all parts of the flap. The wall of the aorta shows three bays, or “ sinuses,” one behind each flap. Hence, when the valve is forced by the rise of pressure in the ventricle, the flap is not flattened back against the wall of the aorta. There is always a certain amount of backwash in the pocket behind it. The instant the pressure in the ventricle begins to fall, the three flaps come together with a click, so smart as to be plainly audible over most of the front of the chest. The click is the “‘ second sound ” of the heart. The auriculo-ventricular valves also make a sound when they close; but this “ first sound of the heart ”’ has a different character. It is prolonged, soft, low-pitched. It is customary to represent the sounds by the syllables “‘ libb dtip—lubb dtp,” the pause during diastole being of about the same length as the sounds when the heart is beating with its normal rhythm. The duration of systole is little affected by variations in the rate of beat. It is diastole that is shortened or prolonged. ‘The second sound is due entirely to the closure of the semilunar valves. It is heard most clearly when the stethoscope is placed over the region where the aorta comes nearest to the wall of the chest—at the second rib cartilage on the right side of the breast-bone. The first sound is loudest near the apex of the heart. It is generally agreed that it is not wholly due to the closure of the auriculo-ventricular valves, but possesses a second constituent. Some persons assert that they can with the ear distinguish the clearer valvular sound at the commencement from the general rumble which overtakes it. The main part of the sound, if it have two constituents, or the whole sound, if there be no dis- tinguishable valvular constituent—observers differ—is just the noise of a distant cab (bruit du cab) or the waves on a far- off beach ; it is the sound which the ear picks up from any irregular mixture of tones which it cannot analyse. It is .. = ig the gis Ca part in adders the first sound cannot be doubted, whether by their first closure or by their subsequent — vibration. We should be inclined to attribute to them the whole performance, were it not that the first sound, or at any rate a sound, is heard during the beating of a bloodless heart. If an animal be killed and the heart removed from its thorax with the utmost despatch, it will beat for about a minute while lying in the palm of one’s hand. When a stethoscope is applied to the ventricle, a “ first sound”’ is heard. This was described as a muscular sound, owing to a misconception. It is similar to the sound which is heard when a stethoscope rests upon a contracting biceps. Until recently the voluntary con- traction of a muscle was believed to be vibratory—a tetanus. The sound corresponds to a rate of about thirty-six vibrations to the second. There being reasons for thinking that muscle contracting naturally does not vibrate as fast as this, the - sound was interpreted as the first overtone of the muscle- note. Muscle was said to vibrate eighteen times a second. The similarity of the first sound of the heart and the ordinary muscle-sound led physiologists to infer that the contraction of the heart also was a tetanus. But this was a mistake. Neither voluntary muscular action nor the contraction of the heart is an interrupted contraction in this sense. In the case of the musculature of the heart especially, contraction is a steady shrinking, followed by a steady relaxation. The sound pro- duced by the bloodless heart is due to the various displace- ments which occur when it contracts. Its interior is very irregular, with its columns, papillary muscles, tendinous cords, valves. The displacement of these various structures is re- sponsible for the noise. The sounds of the heart afford to the physician a means of ascertaining with the utmost nicety the condition of the valves. If the sounds are altered from the normal in the least degree, the valves are not healthy. Alteration of the structure of a valve is in ordinary parlance heart-disease. It is usually indicated by an addition to the normal sound. Such addition is termed a “ murmur ”’; in French, wn bruit de souffle. Hither term is somewhat misleading to the tyro. We remember a fellow-student to whom our chief had in vain expounded the ‘Ugtenins (2 eet Pe et eee 230 — nature of a murmur. “Surely, Mr. S., you can hear the murmur in this case.” We others could hear it as we stood around the. bed. After listening for a minute, S. replied: “T think I could hear it, sir, if the heart wasn’t making such a thundering noise.” The thundering noise was the murmur. It is the business of the physician to recognize that there is a departure from the normal, to analyse its character, to deter- mine the time at which it is heard in relation to the cardiac cycle, and to locate the place on the chest where it is heard most loudly. He is then in a position to state which of the valves is affected and what is the nature of its lesion. Is it a lesion obstructing an orifice, or is it causing regurgitation of blood? Or is one of the valves, as is commonly the case in heart-disease, imperfect in both respects ? A murmur, in the strictest sense, is a sound added to a heart- sound. It is due in all cases to vibration of a fluid column (“ fluid vein’ is the term in physics). When fluid passing ‘under pressure along a tube of a certain calibre enters a tube of smaller calibre, no vibration occurs. When it passes from a tube of smaller calibre into a larger tube or space, it is thrown into vibration. Under normal conditions no vibration occurs in the heart. The auriculo-ventricular orifices are so large that auricle and ventricle form a single cavity when the valve is open. The ventricles drive the blood into tubes of smaller dimensions than themselves. These are not the conditions which set up vibration in a fluid column. But if one of the orifices is constricted, owing to thickening or partial adhesion of its valve, the fluid column vibrates on entering the space beyond it. The sound is propagated forwards, beyond the constric- tion, not behind it, and transmitted to the wall of the ventricle, aorta, or pulmonary artery, as the case may be. When either of the auriculo-ventricular orifices is constricted, the vibration of the fluid column can be felt as well as heard. The finger placed against the chest-wall at the spot where the impulse of the heart occurs is sensible of a thrill. The vibration may occur whilst blood is flowing through an auricle into a ventricle, before the auricle contracts. In time, it is presystolic. The murmur produced byregurgitation into an auricleis synchronous with systole. The murmur due to regurgitation into a ventricle past an incompetent semilunar valve is postsystolic. 231 We have said that the heart is so formed that no vibrating fluid vein is produced when it is functioning normally. Mur- murs are due to alterations in the valves which are visible after death. This statement needs modification. Not in- frequently functional murmurs are heard, which disappear again after a time—in a few weeks, or even days, perhaps. The explanation of murmurs of this class is very difficult. They are heard most frequently in anzmic persons, and appear in these cases to be due to the heart having shrunk, owing to the blood in circulation being deficient in quantity, until the cavities of the ventricles have a smaller diameter than that of the . great arteries into which they expel their contents. Such is the explanation of the physical cause of murmurs given by Chauveau and Marey, the physiologists who have paid most attention to this subject. But it must be remembered that the valves which, when diseased, are the sources of the murmurs are membranous structures. It may be that fluid veins would be produced by them if they were rigid ledges which jutted into the blood-stream ; but, being membranous, they are capable of vibration. Certain physicists are of opinion that a murmur is caused, not by the vibration of a fluid vein, as such, but by the vibration of the membranous structure which impedes the passage of the fluid. The physics of the problem is of little consequence to the physician. The murmur is produced at the spot where a diseased valve is situated, and is propagated forwards. It enables him to ascertain with ~ accuracy what is amiss with the heart. Bloodvessels.—The greater circulation occurs through a closed system of vessels which unite the left ventricle with the right auricle. The aorta gives off lateral branches. Its branches branch. Subdivision continues until the vessels are just wide enough to allow blood-corpuscles to pass in single file, or but little wider. When a bough of a tree divides, the united cross-sections of its twigs, their soft bark being stripped — off, may be a little larger than the cross-section of the bough ; but the disparity is usually small. The united cross-sections of the smaller arteries is considerably greater than that of the trunks which give origin to them. By the time the capillaries are reached,their total bed—their united cross-section—is about 640 times as great as that of the aorta. This estimate is based ~ upon the diminution in the rate at which blood flows throu; h the vessels. The velocity with which a stream flows through % a channel varies as the cross-section of the channel. In a capillary vessel the blood flows at the rate of from 0-5 milli- metre to 1 millimetre per second. In the aorta the velocity is about 320 millimetres per second. In the re-formation of the venous system a converse process of reduction occurs, but not with anything like the same rapidity. The united calibre of the two vene cave, in which the reduction is complete, is about twice that of the aorta. From this it follows that the veins hold much more blood than the arteries ; and since veins are more easily distended, the amount that they can hold varies within wide limits. They constitute to some extent a reservoir for blood. The capillary vessels are the tubes of the circulatory system in which blood comes into use. On the average they are about 0-5 millimetre long. Through them the blood flows slowly. Through their walls alone is there any exchange worth -mentioning between the blood within the vascular system and the lymph by which it is surrounded. Interest therefore centres in these vessels. Their walls are formed of endothelial tiles. In the centre of each thin transparent tile is a boss, where its lens-shaped nucleus is situate. The outline of the tile is sinuous. Its margin dovetails with the margins of those adjacent to it. Oxygen and carbonic acid, nutrient sub- stances and waste-products, pass rapidly through the endo- thelial cells. Leucocytes have the power of pushing the cells aside, in order that they may make their way out of the blood into the lymph which fills the tissue-spaces. With. the ex- ception of the lens and cornea of the eye, cartilage, and the various epidermal structures, all tissues are traversed by capillary vessels. It is not difficult to calculate the number of such vessels in the body exclusive of the liver and the lungs. The diameter of the aorta is 28 millimetres, that of a capillary about 0-008 millimetre. The cross-section of all the capillaries added together is 640 times that of the aorta, as already stated. Many schemata have been devised to illustrate the vascular system ; but all are misleading, inasmuch as they fail to give any idea of the extent to which the subdivision of its vessels is carried. If the water-pipes supplying a town branched ~-e =N oe ~« Bee elise er : 233 until the original conduit was represented by five to six thou- sand million little pipes, the friction which the pumping-station would have to overcome would be very great. But little force would remain in the water when it reached the smallest pipe. Still greater is the resistance to the flow of blood, which is slightly viscous, and contains solid corpuscles, which increase friction. Two thousand miles of capillary tubing in the body of a man, without reckoning the vessels of his liver and lungs ! Water is supplied to houses in rigid tubes. Arteries are elastic, and their elasticity is self-regulating. The cause of this will be apparent if a section of an artery is examined. It ~ Lining | Z Fpitkelium Perforated Elastic Membrane a Musele Ss, Fibres Zee Flastic Fibres FE cut across Fig. 12.—-A PORTION OF THE WALL OF A SMALL ARTERY CUT TRANSVERSELY AND HIGHLY MAGNIFIED. Its inner coat consists of a lining sheet of epithelial scales supported by connective tissue and a strong elastic membrane. This membrane is perforated with holes which place the lymph-spaces on its two sides in continuity. The middle coat is composed of plain muscle fibres and patches of elastic membrane ; the outer coat of elastic fibres, mostly longitudinal, and connective tissue. contains much elastic tissue. It also contains plain muscle- fibres. The smaller the artery, the greater is the amount of muscle relatively to the other constituents of its wall. The wall of a vein contains very little muscle, and not much elastic tissue. The muscle of all arterial walls is in a chronic state of tone. To some extent the degree of tone is varied automatically. Pressure within an artery acts as a stimulus to the muscle- fibres of its wall. Any increase leads the fibres to contract more strongly. Any diminution induces them to relax. The arteries resist distension; they do not narrow to any great extent when pressure falls. But more important than this automatic mechanism for maintaining a uniform pressure in 234 | THE BODY AT WORK the capillaries in general are the changes of pressure in par- — ; a ticular localities, brought about by the mediation of vaso- constrictor and vaso-dilator nerves. In almost all organs and parts of the body the automatic tone of arteries is enhanced by impulses which flow continuously down vaso-constrictor nerves. These impulses start from, or, to speak more accu- rately, pass through, the vaso-motor centre in the medulla oblongata. From every part of the body impulses ascend to this centre, urging it to keep up the blood-pressure by universal constriction. Yet no separate organ would be interested in sending such a message if it were not open to it to ask at the same time that the constriction of its own vessels might be relaxed. Hence it may be said that every individual in the community is crying out for universal economy, with more generous treatment of himself. The response made by the State to the latter part of his demand is in proportion to the vehemence with which it is presented. If the spinal cord of an animal be cut across near the medulla oblongata, respiration being maintained by pumping air into and out of the lungs, the heart continues to beat with un- diminished force, but the pressure in the large arteries falls to one-third of its normal height. Constricting impulses no longer pass down the spinal cord from the vaso-motor centre. This experiment also illustrates the truth of the statement that models of the vascular system—arrangements of pumps and indiarubber tubes—are more likely to mislead than to inform. In an artificial schema the relaxation of the constriction of the small tubes on the proximal side of the capillary vessels would reduce friction. Fluid would reach the capillaries in larger quantity, and pass through them more quickly. The pressure in the tubes which represented veins would consequently ap- proach more nearly to that on the arterial side. But when the spinal cord is divided the pressure falls in the veins, as well as in the arteries. This is due to another factor, and one of very great importance in the regulation of the circulation. The blood from the digestive organs is collected by the “ portal system ”’ of veins. These do not join the inferior vena cava ; they go to the liver, where they again break up into capillaries. It is not until after this second distribution through minute vessels that the blood is re-collected by the hepatic veins and J 1 o- ay oe ee se Be i SES a. a a wih See ee SS ae ie or > = ey ) = URW CIRCULATION 235 _ forwarded to the heart. As in the case of the arteries, the portal system of vessels is controlled by the nervous system. When the spinal cord is divided they also dilate. The whole vascular system becoming more capacious, blood-pressure falls in veins as well as in arteries. When the digestive organs are active, other parts of the body are kept short of blood. It chanced to the writer, in his student days, to spend the early summer in Paris, with a big healthy Yorkshireman as companion. We dined together each night at one of the restaurants of the Palais Royal @ prix fixe. After dinner, with British regularity, my friend called for the Times. Then followed a short period of placid reading, interrupted by the remark : “ How cold it is!’ Half an hour later, giving himself a shake : “‘ Suppose we go and dine somewhere else 2” His well-ordered digestive organs had made short work of the two-franc dinner. They had been ably supported by the vaso- motor system of nerves which provided them with the bulk of the blood, while limbs and skin ran short. Vaso-constrictor nerves leave the spinal cord by the roots (called “rami communicantes”’) of sympathetic ganglia. Beyond the ganglia they apply themselves to the large arteries whose course they follow. The constrictor nerves for the face and neck leave the spinal cord within the chest by the roots of the first four thoracic nerves. They do not at once apply them- selves to the great artery of the head. Until the upper part of the neck is reached, they traverse the ganglionated sympa- thetic cord, which lies behind the carotid artery and internal jugular vein. If in a rabbit this cord be cut, the vessels of its ear dilate, as evidenced by the rosy blush which is observed when a light is held behind it. If the upper part of the sympa- thetic cord be stimulated, the ear grows pale. The redness of the ear remains for many days after section of the nerve; but gradually the engorgement diminishes, and the vessels acquire the power of automatically regulating the flow. The classical experiment with the rabbit’s ear suffices to show the relation of bloodvessels and nerves which holds good for all areas of the skin. The condition of the skin is the chief factor in regulating the temperature of the body. In a cold atmosphere its vessels are severely constricted to limit loss of heat. When one passes into a warm room the constriction is 236 relaxed. The skin is flushed ; heat is thrown off by radiation. The sweat-glands secrete water, which is evaporated by the heat of the skin. Constriction and ‘remission of constriction are the processes which diminish or increase loss of heat. This mechanism is different in the case of glands and some other structures which, when active, require an abundant supply of blood. Such organs are provided with vaso-dilator in addition to vaso-constrictor nerves. The most conspicuous example of this is to be seen in the case of the submaxillary gland. The nerve to this gland runs for some distance as an isolated thread—the chorda tympani. Stimulation of the chorda tympani has the double effect of dilating the arteries of ~ the gland and of causing it to secrete. But the administration of atropin prevents secretion. Vaso-dilation is then the only visible effect. Stimulation may increase sixfold the outflow of blood from the veins of the gland. It rushes through with such rapidity that it retains its bright arterial hue. The gland also receives a twig from the sympathetic cord in the neck, which, as already stated, controls the vessels of the face. By stimulating the one nerve or the other the physiologist can at will increase or diminish the amount of blood flowing through the submaxillary gland. Stimulating any sensory nerve causes in a reflex manner an increased outflow of constrictor impulses from the centre in the medulla oblongata to all parts of the body, with the excep- tion of the part to which the sensory nerve appertains. Its own constituency receives an increased supply of blood. It is not difficult to appreciate the importance of this double action. A part is injured. The restrictions placed upon its supply of blood are suspended. Lest its increased consumption should lead to a general fall in pressure, all other parts have their supply curtailed. The effect is even more pronounced than this. The whole blood-pressure is raised above its ordinary level. The flow of blood to the injured part is therefore greater than it would be were relaxation of its arteries the only change. The most important of all constrictor nerves are the splanchnics which control the supply to the stomach and intestines. When these nerves are cut, the digestive organs become engorged to such an extent that a pronounced fall of the general blood-pressure is the result. Their stimulation renders the digestive organs anemic. We have already shown re et ry ok . : tion of vaso-constriction occurs in a reflex The reflex relaxation of the splanchnic area is a matter of great importance, because it can be brought about by stimulation of one of the sensory nerves of the heart. The higher the blood-pressure, the harder the heart would work if left to itself. It is an impetuous organ, always trying to quicken its pace and to increase the force of its beat. Ex- cessive zeal would get it into trouble if severe precautions were not taken to hold it in check. True, it is encouraged by certain “‘ accelerator nerves ’’—sympathetic filaments which leave the spinal cord by the anterior roots of the second and third thoracic nerves ; but the influence which the accelerators exert under normal conditions is not, it would seem, very pronounced. The nerves which restrain the heart are much more in evidence than those which urge iton. The arrangements for diminishing the work of the heart are of two kinds. In the first place, branches derived from the vagus act as a continuous check. From a certain spot in the medulla oblongata, the cardio-inhibi- tory centre, impulses are always descending to slow the heart. They are of reflex origin, but a high blood-pressure in the centre increases the facility with which they are transmitted. Some of these stimuli originate in the heart itself, ascending and descending the vagus nerve. The remainder come from various sources. A severe injury to any part of the body slows the heart. Injury to the intestines, such as occurs in peritonitis, is particularly effective in increasing vagus inhibition. Slowing of the heart lowers blood-pressure. When both vagi are cut, the heart begins to gallop whatever may be the pressure against _ which it has to work. A sensory nerve of the heart, termed the “ depressor,”’ is the chief agent in lowering ‘blood-pressure. Its course is not the same in all animals, but it runs more or less in conjunction with the vagus. Usually it joins its superior laryngeal branch. Im- pulses which ascend this nerve inhibit the constriction of the splanchnic vessels. They open a floodgate which brings down the general pressure. The severe pain and extreme distress of angina pectoris are the cry of the heart when blood-pressure is too high—when it feels unable to work against it. This was recognized by physiologists long before a remedy was known. A systematic search was instituted for a drug which could be té inch. In most cases the fibres are attached by one end to a bone, by the other to a tendon ; and since they are shorter than the muscle as a whole, the tendon commences as a membrane which covers the surface of the muscle, sloping to it from the bone to which by their other ends the fibres are attached. A fibre is developed from a single cell. The cell elongates, its nucleus divides, and the daughter-nuclei divide until several hundred have been formed; but cell-division does not follow. The result is a cylindrical mass enclosed within a delicate mem- branous sheath, the sarcolemma. In the early stages of its development its nuclei are in the axis of the fibre, but sub- sequently they are displaced outwards. In the most highly specialized muscle, known as the “white” variety, they lie just beneath the sarcolemma (c/. Fig. 16, B). 17—2 be ~~ i yh hss, 260 THE BODY AT WORK The feature of this type of muscle is its transverse striation, — almost mathematically regular. Commonly striated muscle is — spoken of as “ voluntary,” because, for the most part, it is under the control of the Will; but the term, in so far as it implies a connection between structure and mode of actuation, is misleading. Transverse striation is evidence of capacity for — rapid action. The muscles which the Will directs exhibit promptitude; but striated muscle, which is not under the direction of the Will, is found in certain situations—e.g., the upper part of the cesophagus. Conversely, many animals can voluntarily call into action muscle which is not striped. A turkey erects its feathers by setting in motion little groups of ** plain ’”’ fibres, which pull on elastic tendons attached to the tips of the buried ends of their shafts. Plain muscle contracts less promptly and relaxes more slowly than the striped variety. Cardiac muscle is quicker in acting than plain, but does not hold the contraction so long. All striped muscle is not equally rapid. Two varieties are distinguishable : “‘ white fibres,’ which respond suddenly to a single stimulus and quickly relax ;“‘ red fibres,” which respond in a more leisurely way, but remain contracted longer. In some muscles these two types of fibre are intermixed. Others are wholly red or wholly white. Everyone is familiar with the contrast which the white flesh of a turkey or of the domestic fowl presents to the red flesh of game-birds and birds of prey. In the breast of a blackcock a sheet of white muscle overlies a mass of red. When the bird is cooked the difference in colour is strongly marked. Of the two muscles which, in a rabbit, correspond to our muscles of the calf, the superficial, gastroc- nemius, is white; the deeper, soleus, red. The former acts over both knee and ankle joints; the latter over the ankle only. The muscle which, acting over a longer range, has to - contract more quickly is white ; the shorter, more slowly acting muscle is red. Experiment shows that red and white muscles are distinguished by a difference in the promptitude with which they respond to an electric current. It shows, too, that the white muscle is exhausted sooner than the red. It cannot give sO many successive responses to stimulation without a rest. We shall find, when we are considering the minute structure of striped muscle, a difference between its two varieties which MUSCLE . 261 we can correlate with their different modes of action. All - human muscles belong to the red kind. The most efficient muscle-fibres in the animal kingdom are found in insects. This will not surprise anyone who thinks of an insect’s power of movement. If aman could jump as many times his own height as a flea can, he would clear the dome of St. Paul’s. An ant can drag an object sixty times as heavy as itself, with no wheels beneath it to diminish friction. Under the same conditions a horse cannot drag much more than its own weight. A dragon-fly, it is asserted—although we have not met a man who guarantees that he has made the observation— will support its heavy body in the air by the rapid vibration of its wings for four-and-twenty hours without alighting. The chirp of a cricket is produced by the rubbing together of its hind-legs. A mosquito sounds its war-cry much in the same way. The pitch of the note proves that the insect’s muscles are contracting and relaxing at least 300 times a second. None of these figures must be applied without qualifications in estimating the relative strength of insect and human muscle. Weight for weight, the muscle of a flea is not so much stronger than ours as the figures might lead one to infer. To ascertain the numerical relation, it is necessary to compare the total cross-section of the two chief segments of a flea’s leg with the cross-section of the extensor muscles of a man’s thigh and calf, and a man’s weight with the weight of a flea. Neverthe- less, after all deductions have been made, a considerable balance of superiority lies with the insect as regards the strength of its muscles, their rapidity of contraction, and power of repeating - contraction without fatigue. An insect’s muscle is the most suitable that can be obtained for microscopic examination. Its pattern is larger and more distinct than that of other animals. That the pattern should be larger is not quite what might have been expected. It would not have sur- prised us had we found the pattern finer in the more effective type. Nothing is easier than to mount a specimen of insect-muscle. The large water-beetle (Dytiscus marginalis) is an excellent subject. It is so easily handled. Having cut off the animal’s head, a leg is pulled out from the thorax. _ It is split open with a penknife, and a little of the muscle is dug out from within its card case, placed on a “aisast aha and covered * \ cover ‘slip. If the preparation has been made quickly and clea y> I muscle remains alive for five or ten minutes. Not only can it. be studied, with the microscope, unaltered by reagents, but under the most favourable circumstances the progress along — its fibres of waves of contraction can be watched. The — 4 structure of the fibres is more easily made out if a little salt-— a solution or white of egg is added to the preparation. Striped muscle is crossed by bands, dim, bright, and dark. The sequence is as follows: Starting with the very thin dark line, which often appears as a row of dots, the next band is bright ; then comes a dim band about twice as broad as the bright one ; : Reset = Cohnheims Areas--- i ii Ti B Fig. 16.—A, A MINUTE PORTION OF AN INSECT’S MUSCLE-FIBRE, HIGHLY MAGNIFIED. B, WHITE FIBRE OF MAMMALIAN MUSCLE. A, The nuclei are in the core of the fibre. B, The nuclei lie immediately beneath the sarcolemma. The disc on the left of this fibre, and the fibril on its right, show the two ways in which striated muscle-fibres tend to cleave. The dark line, or row of dots, is known as Dobie’s line, or Krause’s membrane. The figures are severely diagrammatic. then another bright band. This sequence is repeated with extreme regularity from end to end of the fibre. Usually the bands cross the whole breadth of the fibre, although occasion- ally it is divided by longitudinal lines into parts in which the stratification is shifted a little backwards or forwards. A segment of a fibre comprises the substance between two dark lines—i.e., two bright bands with a dim one between them. If the muscle has been hardened in one of the fluids commonly used for the purpose of preparing tissues for the microscope, with its two ends fixed, say, by binding them to a piece of a match, so that it could not shrink, a thin clear line appears crossing the middle of the dim band. This seems to show that the fibre is not made up of single dim discs between two bright MUSCLE 263 discs, but of couples, comprising half a dim disc and a bright disc. The thin dark lines indicate that the fibre is divided into compartments by transverse septa, which are probably re- ticulated. The appearance of a transverse line of dots, in place of a continuous line, is due to the existence of very fine longi- tudinal markings (it is unsafe to give them a name which connotes structure). Where the longitudinal lines cross the transverse lines, the optical effect is the appearance of a dot. If pieces of muscle are placed in a solution of osmic acid, they become hard and brittle, and their markings are accentuated. Muscle from the claw of a crab or a lobster is very suitable for this purpose, owing to its exceptional freedom from connective tissue. After this hardening the fibres are easily separated with the aid of needles into fibrils immeasurably slender. An isolated fibril shows with extreme distinctness the alternation of dark, bright, dim, bright, dark markings already described. The appearance of a cross-section of a fibre also proves that it is a bundle of fibrils. The cut ends of the fibrils appear as dots surrounded by homogeneous substance. In this respect there is an important difference between red muscle and white. In the red fibres the fibrils are fewer and thicker than they are in white, and the embedding substance is more abundant. It is generally assumed that the homogeneous substance, sarco- plasm, is the nutrient protoplasm of the fibre, the fibrils the contractile elements. The more complete the differentiation of the fibre into fibrils, the more rapid is its action; the more abundant the sarcoplasm, the greater its capacity for continued work. If a living muscle-fibre is observed while a wave of contraction is passing down it, the ends of the fibre being free, so that its shortening is not prevented, it is noticed that the widening of the fibre is accompanied by the thinning, even to obliteration, of the bright bands. The dim discs extend laterally, without any noticeable diminution of their thickness. It looks as if the bright discs, or something contained in the bright discs, were absorbed into the dim discs. The fibre is, as we have already pointed out, striated longitudinally. The striation is more clearly visible in the dim discs than it is in the bright ones. That the dim disc has an architectural structure absent from the bright disc is placed beyond doubt when a muscle- fibre is illuminated with polarized light. The dim | then found to be doubly refracting ; the bright disc is not. When the prism in the tube of the microscope is placed with — ‘f its axis at right angles to the axis of the prism which inter- _ venes between the source of light and the stage of the micro- _ scope, a succession of bright bands is seen corresponding to the dim bands seen with unpolarized light. The rest of the fibre is invisible, because it has not the property of twisting the undulations of light which the lower prism has set all in the same plane. Various hypotheses as to the cause of con- traction, or, to speak more correctly, as to what happens during contraction, have been based upon the thinning of the bright discs. It is assumed that the dim discs have a definite- ness of structure which the bright discs do not possess. They are thought of as being traversed by pores, or as consisting of short rods. Microscopists who take the latter view believe that during contraction the more fluid substance, sarcoplasm, which occupies the bright bands is drawn into the dim bands between the short rods, or sarcostyles, which are consequently separated more widely. No tissue could be more unsuitable than muscle for micro- scopic examination ; for none other offers the same optical difficulties. This will be evident to anyone who considers the description already given of the markings which it ex- hibits. Whatever may be the true interpretation of these markings, it is clear that they point to an almost infinite multiplication of minute elements adjusted with absolute accuracy side by side and end to end. A cylinder filled with these transparent objects has to be viewed by transmitted light. The elements, whatever may be their nature, refract light in different degrees. It is impossible to eliminate the effects of internal reflection, refraction, and interference of waves of light. The most alluring hypothesis must be accepted with a considerable amount of reserve. Any fact which seems to militate against it must be taken into consideration. The view set forth above, in general terms, is very attractive to everyone who wishes to bring muscle within the category of machines. Suppose we accept the hypothesis that the dim band is a plate made of sarcostyles surrounded by sarcoplasm then the impulse which reaches a fibre causes an alteration in 265 the surface relations of the rods to the substance in which they are embedded. Molecules of fluid from the bright bands are drawn in amongst them; the rods are pushed farther apart ; the fibre broadens with a corresponding diminution in length. This brings muscular contraction into the category of the phenomena which play the most important réle in bringing about the varied activities of the animal mechanism. Con- traction is due to osmosis. The separation of muscle into fibrils after hériining does not seem to bear out either the rod or the pore hypothesis of the structure of the dim disc. It must be remembered, however, that before the fibrils are teased apart the substance of the fibre has been coagulated. The fluid in the bright disc may thus have become as much a part of the fibril as the rod in the dim disc. The longitudinal striation of plain muscle and the appearance of continuous fibrillation in heart-muscle is more difficult to reconcile with the hypothesis that striped muscle is composed of interrupted rods. Muscle transforms the energy supplied to it by the blood into mechanical work. It is doubtful whether any hypothesis as to structure will help us to an understanding of the way in which this transformation is effected. Explanations are seductive, but all attempts at explaining the connection be- tween molecular change and change in shape must be viewed with suspicion. It is quite clear that muscle as a motor is not to be compared with any form of motor with which we are acquainted. It is also clear that the theory of muscle must be applicable to all its varieties—striped, cardiac, and plain. _ It must cover the alterations in form of an amoeba and the streaming movements of protoplasm within a vegetable cell. Probably it must extend farther, and cover the discharge of electricity by an electric organ and the emission of light by the lamp of a firefly. We are on ground so treacherous that we are not sure whether, in crossing it, we may lean with confidence on the laws of thermodynamics ; and doubt as to the applic- ability of these laws to living tissue almost upsets one’s mental balance. Until we have evidence to the contrary, we are bound to exclude such a misgiving from our minds. If we allow it to influence us at all, it is merely to the extent of causing us to hesitate to assume that the explanation of muscular con- 266 THE BODY AT WORK traction can be based upon an analogy between muscle and any known mechanical contrivances for generating power, not even excluding apparatus designed for the purpose of measuring osmotic force. ; If living muscle is frozen, pounded with snow containing 0-6 per cent. of sodic chloride, and placed upon a filter, a fluid plasma passes through the filter as the mixture thaws. Like blood-plasm, it clots spontaneously—without, however, so far as is known the intervention of a ferment. All muscles become rigid after death, owing to the coagula- tion of their plasma. It used to be thought that contraction was a stage towards rigidity—a stage from which muscle, so long as it is alive, recovers. This view was based upon the fact that exhausted muscle—such, for example, as that of a hare which has been coursed—becomes rigid much sooner than rested muscle. But this phenomenon has a different explana- tion. The setting of muscle in rigor mortis is due to the de- velopment of lactic acid (one of the waste-products of active muscle). The more there is of this ready formed at the time of death, the more quickly does coagulation of muscle-plasma occur. The formation of lactic acid is due to deficiency of oxygen. So long as muscle obtains as much oxygen as it wants, its metabolism is complete. The oxidized products which it loses are water and carbonic acid. This is true also of the changes which occur after death. If a strip of muscle is hung in an atmosphere of oxygen, it forms no lactic acid, and it does not become rigid. If, on the other hand, the supply of oxygen has run short before death occurred, rigor mortis sets in very quickly. A dead frog takes a long while in becoming rigid, and its rigidity is transient. Until the moment of death the frog is taking up oxygen through its lungs, and even after death it probably takes it, as it does when it is alive, through the skin. A fish becomes rigid very quickly. For some time after it is caught it continues to live ; but, being unable to breathe in air, every molecule of oxygen which was in its body when it left the water is used up before it dies. In the human body rigor mortis usually sets in from two to four hours after death, and lasts about two days; but both the rapidity of its appearance and its duration depend upon various circumstances. As muscles become rigid they con- MUSCLE ; 267 tract, moving the limbs, and the shortening is more extensive than mere coagulation of muscle-plasm would account for. It is evident that a process similar to functional contraction precedes coagulation. Many a watcher in the chamber of death has been startled by the shaking of the bed. Even a sound resembling a sigh may be caused by contraction of the muscles of the chest. Placing his hand over the region of the heart, the attendant finds the body warmer than it was when life became extinct, for much oxidation has since taken place. What chemical changes occur in muscle when it contracts ? What is the chemical source of its power? Carbonic acid is given off. This is the only product which we can collect and measure ; but it is taken for granted that hydrogen atoms also combine with oxygen, forming water. There is no reason for thinking that nitrogen is removed from the molecules of its protoplasm with any greater rapidity during the activity of muscle than when it is quiescent (cf. p. 212). Is the oxida- tion immediate and complete, or does it occur in stages ? For many years attention has been directed to lactic acid, partly because this substance is found in muscle which has been made to contract under experimental conditions, partly because, on theoretical grounds, glycogen (animal starch) is looked upon as the most important of muscle-foods. Lactic acid—C,;H,O,—has the same percentage composition as glycogen—C,H,,0,. Its formation from glycogen merely involves a rearrangement of atoms. It has been supposed that lactic acid is formed in the first instance, and then, if the supply of oxygen be sufficient, oxidized to carbonic acid and water. But this hypothesis may be resisted on various grounds. Undoubtedly; lactic acid appears when oxygen is deficient. Under all circumstances and in all tissues a certain amount of it is formed. There are reasons for thinking that it carries away the nitrogen which is wasted, as lactamide. But it does not follow that under normal conditions, when muscle is abundantly supplied with blood, lactic acid appears in any greater quantity during activity than during rest. The hypothesis is due to the misconception which we have already endeavoured to correct. It is difficult to get away from the steam-engine analogy. A steam-engine is made of as ae Sia ade Bnet AB ami Bes roe Se En bd ~ . -, a e x . ¢ ja : << 268 THE BODY AT WORK —™” iron and brass. These materials are subject to wear and tear ; but they are not the source of its power. Its power is due to the combustion of fuel. Muscle, physiologists formerly said, is made of protoplasm. This wears down when it works, setting free creatin and other nitrogenous débris. Its fuel is glycogen. This is not the way, however, in which the matter is now regarded. Protoplasm is not the machine only, but also the source of power. Glycogen is not burnt in a frame- work of protoplaam. When muscle contracts, protoplasm casts out CO, and H,O. Glycogen is the food readiest to restore to it the atoms which it has lost. Another consideration opposed to the hypothesis of the conversion of glycogen into lactic acid is the uselessness of such a transformation from a physical point of view. The stability of the atoms of C,H,O, is so little greater than that of the atoms of C,H,,0, that practically no energy is set free when the one substance changes into the other. We cannot, how- ever, overlook the fact that the formation of acid may be a means of profoundly altering the state of the colloid substances dissolved in cell-juice. The casein of milk coagulates when milk turns sour. The neutralization of a faintly alkaline solution of a protein (and muscle is faintly alkaline) will throw it out of solution. The appearance of lactic acid may be intimately associated with movement of protoplasm, and yet the change of glycogen into lactic acid not be the source of the energy which muscle expends. Fatigue.—For its continued activity muscle needs an ade- - quate supply of food and oxygen. Ifthe blood which distributes food is circulating properly, and the liver, the great depot of food, is well stored, fresh supplies are brought to the muscles as they are needed. There are muscles—those of the eye and of the heart, for example—which never become exhausted. However continuous their activity, they take food from the blood as rapidly as they waste it ; a statement which, perhaps, needs qualifying by the addition, “so long as the work exacted of them is such as may be reasonably expected.” If, in a picture-gallery, one keeps the eyes elevated for an hour or more a headache follows. Our eye-muscles have taken over their duties on the understanding that we look down or straight forwards far more often than we look up. If a long-sighted 269 child is required to focus his eyes upon a printed page without the aid of spectacles, not headache merely, but actual disease of the brain, may be the result. The ciliary muscle within the eyeball, which effects accommodation of the eye for near objects, is unduly strained. Even the use of our modern type, with its vertical height greater than its breadth, which has taken the place of square Roman letters, is probably related to the development of astigmatism of the lens, and thus indirectly a cause of headache. It is asserted on high authority that vertical astigmatism, the commonest form, is not present in the eyes of children before they learn to read. Headache is an exaggeration of the feeling of fatigue. It may be inter- preted as the brain’s expression of unwillingness to be made to work ; a protest always to be listened to, notwithstanding that it does not necessarily follow that unwillingness to work is the result of overwork. Constipation, irritation of the sensory nerves of the stomach, overdosing of the brain with alcohol, and many other causes, may, through the vaso-motor system, set up the conditions which normally result from activity un- duly prolonged. The fact that a central disturbance, headache, results from undue muscular work calls our attention to the double nature of the mechanism concerned in movement. Muscles are set in motion through the intervention of the nervous system. After they have worked to an unusual extent the nerve-centres connected with them grow tired. This, at least, is a legitimate inference from the fact that headache occurs when certain muscles of the eyeball have been subjected to an improper strain. But it must be remembered that the muscles of the eyeball never tire. They do not, like other voluntary muscles, give notice that they are in need of rest. It is not so clear that the central mechanism is in any way involved in the fatigue which is produced by excessive use of arms or legs. The muscles of the limbs (and the central nervous system) are protected by the sensations which originate in muscles when they are overworked. The fact that a weary man can, if a great emergency demands activity, use his muscles with as much vigour as if he were fresh from bed, has been cited as an argument in favour of the view that fatigue is of central origin; but it is an argument which works both ways. A strong emotion causes a fervent response from the nervous system. ‘Tired muscles contract energetically wh the impulses which reach them are sufficiently urgent. ae Nothing so definitely removes muscle from the category of _ machines as its liability to fatigue. To speak of a muscle as tired is, of course, to transfer to an object a term which is applicable only to a phenomenon of consciousness ; but it is necessary, unless a cumbrous expression is to be used, to designate thus the effect upon the muscle of prolonged activity. The petrol may be low in the tank, but the quantity burnt in the cylinder at each stroke is not reduced. If an isolated muscle is repeatedly stimulated by an electric current of a certain strength, the response which it makes improves for © the first two or three induction shocks; then it begins to weaken. At each succeeding spasm the muscle shortens a trifle less than before. More remarkable than the diminution in the amount of work done by a muscle which is growing tired is the prolongation of the time taken both in contracting and in relaxing. Further, it has been shown that the fatigue which accompanies the contraction of an isolated muscle is not a condition dependent upon the shrinking of the store of energy which it possessed when it was first thrown into activity. Muscles undisturbed as to blood-supply, and contracting under the direction of the Will, also exhibit it. Speaking generally, it may be said that the tiring of muscle is not so much due to the exhaustion of its store of food as to accumulation of products of action. Vigour is restored to a tired muscle by passing through its bloodvessels a stream of salt-solution, which brings it no food, but washes away some of its waste. But the problem is far more complex than this. The machinery is not simply clogged with the products of its own activity. If the blood of a tired animal is injected into the vessels of one that is rested, the muscles of the latter exhibit the phenomena of. fatigue. Evidently muscle is self-protective. During activity it prepares a “‘fatigue-substance’”’ which poisons its own nerve-endings, making them worse conductors from nerve to muscle of the commands which descend from the brain. Not only does the fatigue-substance dull the nerve-endings in the particular muscle which has contracted, but, being dis- tributed by the blood to the whole body, it produces a general effect. If the legs have been severely worked, they exhibit MUSCLE 271 _ fatigue in the highest degree ; but after a long walk the arms also are less ready and less capable than the state of their nutrition warrants. The condition of stiffness experienced for a day or two after excessive exercise is due to various causes in combina- tion. The fact that it may be remedied by encouraging the circulation through the muscles most affected, as by hot baths and massage, tempts us to assign it also in large measure to accumulation of products of action; but the means taken to reduce stiffness favour the nutrition of the muscles both by giving them more food and by carrying off their waste. Equally remarkable with the self-protective disposition of muscle, which forbids it to give, except at the instance of increasingly urgent messages from the central nervous system, more than a part of the work of which it is capable, is its preparation for meeting an increased demand. It grows with use. Running increases the girth of the leg by developing especially the muscles of the calf. Raising weights enlarges the muscles of the shoulder and arm. Use-growth may reach inconvenient proportions. Nothing is more noticeable during the training of young athletes, whose nutritive responsiveness is at its height, than their liability to pass through a stage in which they are “‘ muscle-bound.” Their legs grow bigger, but their pace falls off. The development by means of exercises of a strong muscular system has received much attention during recent years. Our ancestors cultivated strength and agility in certain movements without paying much attention to the muscles by which the movements were performed. It is fashionable nowadays to lay stress upon the importance of maintaining an abundant musculature, because of its relation to general ** fitness.” The balance between muscular activity and the organic functions which is observed by everyone who takes an active holiday proves beyond doubt that the nutritive con- dition of the various glands and of the heart and bloodvessels is in some degree dependent upon the condition of the muscles. Possibly they secrete into the blood other “ messengers ”’ in addition to fatigue-substance—messengers whose call wakes up the organs of digestion. The man who is so fortunate as to be able to use his muscles in the open air has no need of i at + ee as > ee a 972 THE BODY AT WORK exercises in his bathroom. Failing out-of-door opportuni po: ai much can be done by the systematic use of the various muscles working against resistance. It is alleged, and we are not dis- posed to dispute the justice of the contention, that movements made with the fullest degree of mental concurrence have a more rapid effect upon the growth of muscle than actions more or less unconscious. Muscle and nerve are parts of a single mechanism. It may be that fixing the attention on an exercise, and watching its performance in a looking-glass, aids the nutrition of muscles by increasing the influence of their nerves, possibly by improving the nutrition of their nerve-centre. Unfortunately, this is one of many theories which hardly come within the reach of a control-experiment. Could one concen- trate attention on the movements of the right arm, then absent- mindedly repeat them with equal vigour with the left, it might be possible to ascertain whether there is anything in this idea. Two other contentions with regard to the best way of perform- ing movements, with a view to the promotion of muscular growth, appear to be justified by their results. Working against a moderate or light load is said to be more effective than putting muscles to a severe strain. A small number of maximal contractions, it is said, induce more rapid growth than many partial shortenings. According to this scheme, when a particular muscle needs strengthening, because in a certain action it is to be the chief performer, it is made to bring its two ends as near together as the plan of its attachments allows. Maximal shortening is apparently favourable to blood-supply and otherwise promotes nutrition. Tone.—Hitherto we have spoken of quiescence and activity, as if muscle were doing nothing when not visibly contracting. A wrong impression may be engendered by these terms. Muscle is never idle. During sleep, and still more when a person is under the influence of anesthetics, the muscles approach the condition of machines at rest. But again the language of the workshop is inapplicable. When a headless frog is hanging from a hook its legs are slightly bent. All its muscles are weakly contracted, if we understand by contraction a condition in which the length of muscle is less than it would be were it not alive. But the flexors are tenser than the extensors, hence the crooking of hip, knee, and ankle. If the sensory “MUSCLE 273 roots of the sciatic nerve are cut, the leg straightens out. So long as the nerve was intact the weight of the limbs acted as a stimulus to sensory nerve-endings, causing a reflex “tone”’ of the flexor muscles via the spinal cord. The tone of the extensor muscles was less because they were not stretched by the weight of the limbs. Every joint is under the influence of antagonistic muscles which are perpetually watching one another. When the limb is extended the flexors become anxious. When it is flexed the extensors get ready for a spring. Only when it is half flexed is there anything approaching toa truce. And this in most cases is the position of greatest comfort. But even when most at rest, muscles still possess a certain degree of tone. The tendency to shortening in one set causes it to pull against, and thereby increases the tone of, its opponents. When a muscle contracts it does not lift a loose bone. It has to overcome the tone of the muscles which would cause a movement in the opposite direc- tion. And here another adjustment comes into play. The same gross stimulus which leads to the contraction of A starts impulses of a finer kind for B, directing it to relax its tone. We have seen how the heart and bloodvessels are under the influence of two sets of nerves of opposite sign—anabolic, diminishing irritability ; katabolic, increasing it. All muscles are under similar management ; but we can rarely detect the influence of the anabolic, inhibitory nerves, the brakes, because the katabolic display is overwhelmingly conspicuous. We must be content with two experimental demonstrations. An animal’s hamstrings have been cut ; the flexor muscles of its thigh are therefore severed from their attachments below the knee. The tone of the extensor muscle keeps this joint ex- tended. If now the pad of the foot be tickled, the flexor muscles contract, just as they would do if they were still able to carry out the reflex action of raising the foot. They cannot do this, because their tendons are divided ; nevertheless, the knee bends owing to reflex relaxation of the extensor muscles. Still more striking evidence of reciprocal contraction and relaxation is afforded by the claw-muscles of a crayfish. A weak stimulus to its nerve causes the claw to set open; astronger stimulus causes it to close. Both these movements are due, not to “ contraction,” but to change of tone. Under certain con- 18 O74 THE BODY AT WORK 7~— ditions, a current passed through its abductor muscle, the claw _ being open at the time, causes closure by inhibiting the tone of — this muscle. In this case the stimulus acts directly on the © muscle, producing an effect which is opposite to the one we are accustomed to associate with stimulation ; in place of con- traction, relaxation. Contraction of muscle, moving something, impresses one as a positive phenomenon. Relaxation seems negative—the un- doing of contraction—and to a very large extent this attitude of mind is justified. Return of a muscle to its full length is due either to stretching by the weight it has lifted, or to the an- tagonism of other muscles. An isolated muscle lying on a pool of quicksilver does not return to its full length after it has contracted. But it is necessary to banish the machine idea. A machine gives out all the energy it has in store. Muscle is extremely parsimonious. No stimulus can induce it to part with more than a fraction of its energy. Recovery is as definite a function as disturbance. A machine starts when a crank is moved, stops when it is replaced. Muscle has a certain degree of automatism, although its tendency to act on its own account has been almost completely transferred to the governing nervous system. Muscle and nerve work together, and the efficiency of muscle depends upon the main- tenance of its relations with its nerve. If the nerve is cut, the muscle atrophies. We will not stop to consider whether wasting may be properly attributed to disuse, or to vaso- motor changes. In its lowest form nervous influence shows itself in the regulation of the nutrition of muscle. A somewhat more forcible exhibition of control is seen in the regulation of tone. The maximum is reached when a wave of undoing which has passed down a nerve infects the protoplasm of muscle with the same tendency to disintegration. The muscle- substance explodes. The muscle shortens. Remarkable evidence of the existence of muscle-tone is afforded by the knee-jerk. Place a person on an upright chair, with his legs crossed, muscles lax, foot hanging free. With a paper-knife or the end of a stethoscope, or even the hand used edgewise, tap the ligament which connects his knee- cap with his shin. The tap is instantly followed by a jerking forward of the foot. The deep muscles of the thigh, vastus, ~_- P ; @ ! - 1 MUSCLE 86 and crureus, have contracted. This phenomenon is easy to account for. When we are standing upright, the trunk is supported on three joints, of which one—the hip—is a perfect ball and socket, and the other two—knee and ankle—are of the same order so far as the absence of any provision for lock- ing them is concerned. If the muscles on the front and the back of the leg did not constantly adjust our balance, by swaying the trunk forward when it falls back, and pulling it back when it sways forward, the joints of the leg would double up beneath us. A photographer knows how little confidence is to be placed in a man’s assertion that he is able to stand still. This see-saw of alternate contraction and. re- laxation is kept up by means of nerve-impulses which ascend from the nerve-endings surrounding the separate bundles of tendnos, or from the Pacinian bodies which are found in abundance in the neighbourhood of tendons and ligaments, or from the elaborately twisted nerve-fibres found in muscle- spindles, or possibly from all three classes. Muscle and tendon are richly supplied with sense-organs susceptible to pressure and stretching. There is an abundance of nerve-endings to choose from. The slightest change in their tension, whether due to the muscle’s own contraction or to the action upon it of other muscles or weights, is recorded not only in the spinal cord, but also in the cortex of the cerebellum, and, if the con- traction is an act of volition, in the cortex of the great brain. Although it was skin which was tapped, skin-nerves have nothing to do with the jerk. It was the result of the slight sudden stretching. In short, the tone-mechanism has been fooled. Notice the position of the leg. The knee is semiflexed ; the foot is hanging free. There is nothing for the extensor muscles of the thigh to do. Now, if ever, they are justified in dozing. It is not to be wondered at that the sudden stretching of the ligament takes them off their guard, or that on waking they give a quite unreasonable start. The phenomenon is, as we asserted, easy to account for. It would also be easy to ex- plain, if it were not for the extreme rapidity with which the jerk follows the tap. The interval is about one-hundredth of a second. This is thought to be too short to allow an impulse to ascend a sensory nerve, pass through the cord, and descend a motor nerve. It is true that these reflexes of adjustment 18—2 vz ee Ce eee A ee } oD Se ee Ree ae Se Cah —- “s , 7 She > hate th 276 THE BODY AT WORK must stand on a different level to other reflexes. The tone- impulses which cause them are incessantly patrolling to and fro from sense-organs to nerve-endings. The paths they follow must be the most open in the nervous system. Receptors and effectors must, in an electrician’s phrase, be incessantly switched on ; or, to express the analogy more accurately, the flexor and extensor tone mechanisms are incessantly and reciprocally switching each other on and off. It must be con- fessed that it is very difficult to explain the knee-jerk if it be not a reflex action, but, as has been supposed, a direct response of the thigh muscles to their own stretching. The latter hypothesis does not appear to be reconcilable with its depen- dence upon the maintenance of the nervous connection of the muscles with the spinal cord. It cannot be elicited unless the “‘spinal arc”’ is intact. It ceases after the severance of either sensory or motor roots. Nor will it occur if the supply of blood to the lower end of the spinal cord has been cut off. Still more difficult is it to explain its extraordinary sympathy with everything that happens in the whole nervous system, if the impulses which cause it do not pass through the spinal cord. By a very simple mechanical arrangement it is possible to record the amplitude of the knee-jerk. The foot moves a lever which writes on a travelling surface. The jerk is elicited by the hammer of a clock strapped to the shin. In this way it is possible to extend the period of observation over several consecutive hours, the subject becoming completely oblivious of the movement his foot is making once a second, if it be screened from his view. In deep sleep the jerks stop; but the subject may doze, and still jerk follows tap.. And the record made by his foot mirrors all the changes in his nervous system. If he clench his fist, the movement is reinforced, as it is when a child cries, a lamp is lighted, his ear itches. There is music in an adjoining room. His foot is the baton which beats fortissimo to Wagner, and is lulled to giano by the ‘* Lieder ohne Worte.”” On a bright day this spinal pulse throbs gaily. It is indolent in dull, depressing weather. The knee-jerk is the physician’s guide to the condition of the nervous system. Elasticity of Muscles.—Muscles are very extensible, and after stretching return to their original length. Their elasticity is a quality of great practical importance. It enables them to - MUSCLE | 877 meet sudden resistance without rupture, as when a man alights from a height. At the moment when the feet touch ground elasticity dissipates the shock. The stretching of the muscles then leads reflexly to the increase of their tone. Here we see an advantage in the short reaction-time of the knee- jerk. Tone comes into play long before impulses generated by contact of the sole of the foot with the ground have had time to reach the’ brain, or even to induce reflex contraction through the spinal cord. The elasticity of the muscles is also of use in the performance of certain sudden actions. A pea is flicked across the room by pressing the thumb-nail against the pad of a finger, or a finger against the thumb, and releasing it with a jerk. An electrical change accompanies an impulse in its passage down a nerve, and a wave of contraction in its passage along a muscle. In 1788 Galvani observed that the hind-limbs of a frog, suspended by a metal hook to metal railings, twitched when the wind blew them against the bars. The hook passed through the lumbar plexus of nerves. He recognized that the cause of the twitch was the closing of a circuit. The birth of dynamic or galvanic electricity dates from this observation ; and ever since this phenomenon was first observed the electric changes in nerve-muscle preparations made from frogs’ legs have been favourite subjects of research. Many obvervations with regard to nerve-conduction and muscle-contraction may be made, and many experiments performed, without special apparatus. A frog having been killed by cutting off its head, or by placing it beneath a tumbler with a wad of cotton-wool soaked in chloroform, the skin of the leg is removed, displaying the khaki-coloured muscles, bluish tendons, and bright white threads of nerve. A stretch of the largest nerve of the back of the thigh, the sciatic, is isolated. All the muscles of the thigh are then cut away and the bone nipped across just above the knee. The bones below the knee are removed, the superficial muscle of the calf, the gastrocnemius, being allowed to hang free, its bifid end attached to the fragment of thigh-bone. Its lower end terminating in the tendo Achillis, with its insertion into the prominence of the heel, is left intact. The bone is fixed in a clamp. A light lever made from a wooden spill is suspended from the tendo Achillis. The nerve may then be stimulated in various ways : by crushing in a pair of forceps, F ee Sr aces Ge Se os iz 278 THE BODY AT WORK burning with a heated needle, touching with a drop of t ptyoertl or a strong solution of salt. But of all methods of stimulation, the best is the current from an induction coil. Since it does not injure the nerve, it can be applied as often as may be desired. The amateur provided with an induction coil is in a position to study the relation between stimulus and re- sponse. He can vary the strength of the stimulus and vary the weight which the muscle has to lift. He can observe the progressive onset of fatigue, and otherwise gain much infor- mation regarding the behaviour of muscle as an isolated piece of apparatus. It is the ambition of the expert to obtain absolutely correct records of the time-phases and of the changes in electric. potential of nerve and muscle under varied experimental con- ditions. For this purpose he needs the finest apparatus which instrument-makers can furnish, and the knowledge and dex- terity requisite for its employment. Consider, for example, the record of the change of form. A nerve-muscle preparation, . obtained by the method already described, is arranged so that the point of the lever scratches on a rapidly travelling blackened surface. As the muscle contracts it makes a “tracing.” eS 2s BE a ee ee ee eee, ee ee AP 300 THE BODY AT WORK dendrite seems to be irreconcilable with the hypothesis that a they are disposed in the lines of conduction. In common with those of various other types of neurone, the dendrites of spinal motor cells are beset with “ thorns.” These projections are not rugosities or serrations, but short, delicate threads which stand out at right angles from the dendrites (cf. Fig. 1). About a dozen years ago, the writer made a careful investigation of these structures; at a time when most anatomists regarded them as artifacts. He found that their claim to be regarded as parts of the neurone is as good as that of its axon or its dendrites ; although never seen on certain types of cell, the thorns, of cells which carry them, are perfectly definite in arrangement and spacing. In some kinds of cell they are more numerous, in others less. Neuro-fibrille, as we now know them, had not been discovered at the date when this investigation was undertaken; but on various grounds the conclusion was arrived at that thorns are the cell-ends of fibrils which pass from the end-twigs of arborizing axons into dendrites. Upon this conclusion was based an hypothesis of conduction which is here submitted, not because there is not much to be said against it—or, at any rate, many a hiatus in knowledge to be filled—but because it happens to be the writer's own. The chrome-silver and methylene-blue methods which reveal the existence of thorns do not stain neuro- fibrille. They colour the soft protoplasm in which fibrils are embedded. By modifying the chrome-silver method in every way which still allows a result to be obtained, it was found that thorns sometimes appear as comparatively long slender fila- ments, at others as shorter filaments ending in minute knobs, or as filaments bearing two or three dots ; or finally no filaments are visible, but the dots are in the position which they would occupy if fibrils were present, but not stained. From this it was argued that the soft protoplasm which during life surrounds the filament as a continuous film, either falls back towards the cell after death or is made to shrink into the cell by re- agents. This accounts for the appearance of rod and knob. What is supposed to happen may be illustrated by dipping a wire in treacle. At first, when the wire is withdrawn, it is surrounded with a film. Then the film gathers into droplets. It was sug- gested that the entrance of impulses into dendrites, their con- yA ‘ak le ee were Sen a ne ae et are Th ae Figg THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 301 duction across the space which separates the end-twigs of axons from the dendrites into which their impulses pass, is by means of the thorns, although the thorns are not in them- selves conductors. Conduction occurs only when films of cytoplasm surround the thorns. The first effect of impulses is to call out the films, in the same kind of way that a cur- rent of electricity converts a row of falling drops into a con- tinuous stream. A succession of impulses, by adding to the number of the filaments which are enveloped in cytoplasm, or by increasing the amount of cytoplasm investing certain groups of filaments, increases the openness of the path. Sleep is a condition in which all paths are open. Hence no impulses are effective. Wakefulness, alertness, depends upon the closing of all paths save those which are actually in use. We may go further. The power of concentrating attention is the power of limiting the spread of nerve-impulses in the brain. Alcohol opens extra paths ; the concentrated effort which was making progress with a problem becomes more diffuse. The first effect appears in greater brilliance of thought, gained at some sacrifice of cogency. Unexpected analogies are dis- covered. Imagination takes a wider range. But as the dose is increased, a condition akin to sleep is set up. Nerve-im- pulses become ineffective because, many paths being open, they do not attain a sufficient intensity in any set of paths. These few illustrations are given for the sake of showing the need of a theory of the opening and closing of paths. It is not sug- gested that they favour the particular hypothesis here set forth as to the structural arrangement which provides the paths and regulates their accessibility. Recent discoveries in the finer structure of the central nervous system have provided many problems which at present appear insoluble. One of the discoveries most difficult to make use of in constructing theory is the existence of extracellular or peri- cellular nets, which have the appearance of extraordinarily delicate cases of wire-netting immediately surrounding the nerve-cells. It is somewhat remarkable that the spacing of the nets is often very similar to, if not identical with, the spacing of thorns. While some anatomists look upon the nets as nervous, others regard them as pertaining to the con- nective tissue of the nervous system. At present it is not ies ee eee eer ey Oe ees Cee oe kee eee eS Aa Paes _ > = ‘ > + ‘en, . 4, 7s =~ o> “ Nenaeee a i ae te = = q es a a ee a Ch Plat 302 ‘THE BODY AT WORK known how impulses get across from the finest visible wins of _ arborizing axons to the dendrites of the neurones which they influence. The wealth of structural detail which recent re- search has revealed is an embarrassment to anyone who tries to devise a scheme. Not improbably, pericellular nets are intermediate factors in the exchange; or, if not the nets, the structures whose existence is indicated by the appearance of the nets. In the case of many of the finer markings which staining methods bring into view, it is impossible to say whether they indicate the presence during life of the structure as it appears to be, or whether the markings are due to coagulation of plasma or to strain caused by shrinkage in coagulating agents. In a sense this is not of much consequence. Coagu- lation in a uniform pattern would mean the existence of an architectural substructure which determines the pattern. We may be looking at the cake or at the tin the cake was baked in. There is a danger of seeing too much in a nerve-cell when examining it under the highest powers of the microscope, and of » endeavouring to picture in too much detail the arrangements which regulate the flow of impulses. Its markings are so com- plicated as to suggest to the mind of the observer that it is itself a microcosm—a nervous system in miniature. Neuro- fibrillee appear to offer many alternative paths within the cell. It is unlikely that such a way of looking at the unit of struc- ture is the right one. A certain motor cell of the spinal cord is connected by its axon with thirty or forty separate muscle- fibres ; but there is no reason for thinking that the fibres ever contract save as a single group. The axon consists of parallel fibrille, but these do not appear to be needed as separate con- ductors ; an impulse travels down the fascicle. It does not appear to be necessary in the case of a motor cell, and pre- sumably the statement holds good for the large cells of the cerebellum and cerebrum to picture any arrangement for the simultaneous conduction in its axon of several impulses, or for the conduction of one impulse along one of its fibrille and a different one along another. What is necessary is that this particular efferent path Z should be accessible from every other part of the nervous system—from A to Y. If, merely for the sake of filling the space which would otherwise be blank in the mental picture, we imagine a pericellular net connected THE NERVOUS SYSTEM .—~—=s303 by thorns with the body and dendrites of the nerve-cell Z, then the net is the meeting-ground of all the routes through which Z is called into action. A nerve-wave from any of the neurones A to Y, breaking upon this net, passes along the thorns into the protoplasm of Z. In size a granule of the cerebellum presents a marked con- trast to a motor cell of the spinal cord ; yet it is formed on essentially the same plan. From its minute round body (about 8 » in diameter) four or five slender dendritic processes are drawn out. Each dendrite ends in a little bunch of twigs, resembling fingers curved over the palm. Its single slender axon runs towards the surface of the cortex. As the granules lie at a considerable depth, this course is, for those which dis- tribute to the most superficial layers, a long one. They pass from the granular to the molecular layer between the big cells of Purkinje. When the axon has reached a certain level in the molecular layer, it divides into two threads which run for a great distance, right and left. The granules of the cerebellum have a curious developmental history. Every neurone in the body has a lifelong existence. Except for the rare accident of its destruction by disease it occupies its station to the hour of death. But at the time of birth many neurones are still immature. Not all the granules of the cerebellum have yet assumed their permanent form or situation. Beneath the pia mater there is still a layer of minute undifferentiated cells. These, as they grow into granules, elongate, in the first instance, into long spindles. Subsequently they sink down through the molecular layer and between the cells of Purkinje, leaving the poles of the spindle as the right and left divisions of the axon (Fig. 21). It is interesting to learn that such a migration is possible. It is also of interest to find that a tiny granule of the cere- bellum goes through the same stages in attaining its adult form as one of the large cells of a spinal ganglion. There are many different types of neurone. Any attempt to describe them, or to give an account of the various details of structure which recent improvements in technique have enabled anatomists to observe, would fill a lengthy treatise ; and would, moreover, be beside our aim, which is limited to | obtaining such an idea of the unit of the nervous system as will it “will be evident that enatomiets are pedir to an understanding of the mechanism. It will also be evident that they have already more information than they can apply. | . They are cognizant of many details of structure which they cannot interpret in terms of function; and at the same time __ are aware of wide gaps in their knowledge regarding facts Se eee |~Pia mater 0529 Onn , Oo Fic. 21.—THE GROWTH AND MIGRATION OF GRANULES OF THE CEREBELLUM. Half a dozen nuclei of as yet undeveloped granules are seen lying beneath the pia mater. From this level to the bottom of the drawing granules are shown in successive stages of growth. These developing granules, selected from various preparations of the cortex of the cerebellum, were drawn from nature. which are essential to the construction of any scheme. This much is clear: A sense-cell on the surface or beneath it is touched (probably entered) by the ultimate twig of the outer limb of a neurone whose cell-body lies in a spinal ganglion, while its inner limb, as a fibre of a posterior root, enters the spinal cord. In the spinal cord the root-fibre splits into an ascending and a descending division which rain branches into the grey matter over a considerable area above its point of —, ~\ ,and a smaller area below it. The finest twigs of these branches are to be seen in the vicinity of the cell-bodies and _ dendrites of certain other neurones. The axons of these second links arborize in a similar way in the vicinity of large motor cells, whose axons in turn become fibres of anterior roots. (For sim- plicity’s sake no reference is made to hosts of other neurones which link the ganglion-cell and the motor-cell to other cells higher in the cord or brain.) An impulse generated in the sense-cell on the surface of the body runs up the root neurone into the cord, where the ultimate twigs of the posterior root fibre offer it a wide choice of distribution. Following the path of least resistance, it passes into neurone No. 2. Again, the arborization of No. 2 offers it alternative paths. It makes a choice which lands it in No. 3. No. 3 passes the impulse on to the muscle-fibres with which it is connected. Three points are especially worthy of attention: (1) The impulse has a wide (literally, an unlimited) choice of routes. The skin of the finger is touched. Any muscle may respond, although resistance is so graded as to cause the impulse to seek in the first instance the group of muscles which is most often required to act in consequence of stimulation of the finger. This means, we may suppose, that it follows the chain which, having the smallest number of links, offers least resistance. If it cannot get through to these muscles, owing to the fact that other impulses, acting simultaneously, either increase the resistance in this particular path, blocking its way, or reduce the resistance in an alternative path, it spreads farther afield. (2) Owing to the ramification of the root-fibre which conveys it to the cord, an impulse is not limited to a single line of dis- tribution. It reaches many secondary links. It may therefore influence various effector neurones simultaneously. For ex- ample, a stimulus which calls extensor muscles into action, at the same time inhibits their flexor antagonists. (3) The path which it finally takes is accessible to all other impulses. Its root neurone was peculiar to itself. Link No. 2 was more or less a common path. Neurone No. 3 is open to every impulse which traverses the nervous system. Anatomy justifies the construction of the scheme just out- lined. But there are many points regarding structure upon which a physiologist desires information, many details that he 20 THE BODY AT WORK wants to see filled in. How is the impulse passed from the arborization of axon No. 1 to the dendrites of neurone No. 22 — By what structural arrangement is resistance introduced, and how is it regulated, if it varies ? Supposing the resistance to __ be higher in one path than in another, or supposing that more ~ force is needed to enable an impulse to invade a wider field, how is additional energy supplied? To the first question no answer can be given at present—the mechanism by which impulses are transferred from one neurone to another is un- known ; yet it is convenient to find a name for the junction of axon-endings and dendrites. It is termed a “synapse,” on the understanding that the word involves no hypothesis as to its structural nature. It is generally held that resistance is introduced into nerve-circuits at synapses ; although this again is a provisional statement. The phenomena for the explana- tion of which the idea of synaptic resistance was introduced, may be accounted for on a purely anatomical basis of distribu- tion. The extent to which one neurone influences another may depend upon the size of the brush of fibrils with which its axon touches it. Ifa certain force is needed to discharge a neurone, a nerve-current must either have a sufficiently high potential when it reaches it, or it must act upon it for a sufficient length of time. There is little to choose between the arguments which place the resistance at the synapse and those which transfer it to the nerve-cell body. As a mechanism the nervous system is unthinkable, unless we picture its units as independent, yet capable of forming associations ; as functionally discrete, yet entering into func- tional continuity. When acting, they act as chains. Impulses run from link to link, from the end-twigs of an axon of one cell to the dendrites of the next. Neurones are so arranged as to make it impossible for impulses to escape backwards out of © dendrites into axon-twigs. In this respect the system is valved. But there is no reason for thinking of the substance of the neurone as polarized in any way. The physical accom- paniment of an impulse—the electric variation—travels with equal facility up and down its axon. There is no evidence of any specificity of neurones ; on the contrary, it is clear that impulses of every kind—that is to say, from every source, for we recognize no specificity of im- THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 307 pulses —can travel equally well through neurones of all forms. At every junction, in passing through each synapse, they are delayed. It takes at least 0-01 second (less if the knee-jerk be a true reflex action) for a message delivered to the cord by a sensory root to reach a motor root. This hundredth of a second—the sum of the delays entailed in fording two or three synapses—is regarded as the minimum reflex time. To it must be added, in considering any particular reflex action, the time taken in travelling up sensory and down motor nerves. Delay indicates resistance. If a sensory stimulus be not sufficiently pronounced to provoke a reflex action, the reflex may be obtained on intensifying it. Prolonging or re- peating the stimulus—really the same thing, since sensory impulses are rhythmic, not continuous—has a far more potent effect than increasing its force. The resistance of synapses gives way after a number of impulses have bombarded them. The desire of brushing a fly from the skin, if resisted, becomes intolerably urgent after a time. A persistent outflow of im- pulses produced by the irritation of a spot in the cortex of the brain overwhelms the nerve-muscle system in an epileptic fit. The following is an experiment illustrating the spread of im- pulses from their customary path to another less often used : A piece of blotting-paper, wet with vinegar, is placed on the inner side of the thigh of a brainless frog. There is no use in trying the experiment on a frog which retains its brain; the substitution of one action for another would be an exhibition of the adaptation of means to end—a demonstration of the -animal’s right of choice. Besides, the frog might choose not to act, and so the experiment would fail. The brainless frog wipes off the blotting-paper with the foot of the same side. This foot is then fixed so that the action cannot be performed, and the blotting-paper replaced. After a longer interval the frog removes it with its other foot. Evidently it is more difficult for the impulses generated by the irritation which the vinegar causes to get across the cord than it is for them to reach motor neurones on the same side. Evidently, too, the con- tinued irritation of the vinegar adds to the travelling power of the impulses. They are strengthened until they are capable of overcoming the resistance in the longer path. “ Resistance in conductors”’ and “ potential of current ”’ are terms with 20—2 308 THE BODY AT WORK which the study of electricity has rendered us familiar; but it must be evident from the experiment just described that — these terms are not really applicable to nervous phenomena, convenient though they may be for use in an allegorical sense. Holding the foot does not, by any mechanism which we can recognize, switch off the shorter circuit, yet the impulses abandon it for the longer path. There is no evidence of a struggle to free the foot that has been fixed, coincident with the spread of impulses, as they gather sufficient strength to reach the nervous mechanism of the other leg. The right foot not being available, the impulses choose the route to the left foot. Any attempt to explain this in terms of resistance and potential involves the formulation of a number of subsidiary hypotheses ; easy to devise, no doubt, but stultifying to the explanation exactly in proportion as they complicate it. Yet the hypothesis of lines of greater and of less resistance (keeping as far away from electrical analogies as possible) is essential to any explanation of nervous phenomena, and is, moreover, justified by the evidence available. There are two causes in chief upon which it depends: (1) The greater the number of neurones in a linear chain, the greater is the number of synapses to be traversed. If A, B, C are in the same circuit, the sum of their resistance has to be overcome. (2) The greater the number of neurones amongst which a nerve-current has to be subdivided, the smaller the charge available for each of them. Imagine rvs so placed as to divide B and (C, the charge delivered by A between. This arrangement has, probably, an anatomical expression which accounts for the relative ease or difficulty of a path, even on the supposition | that impulses do not open out as they advance—do not spread along all the branches into which an axon divides—but keep to a given line. The axon of neurone A divides, to branch about B, C, and D; but its representation in the several pericellular nets (the expression may pass for the sake of the simplicity which it introduces into the picture) is unequal. In the vinegar experiment the impulses delivered to the spinal cord by the root-ganglion neurone A pass to neurone B of the posterior horn. B’s axon arborizes more freely about the cell-body of neurone C in the anterior horn of the same ow a. > ree pay a i THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 309 side than it does about neurone D in the anterior horn of the opposite side. Hence the impulses generated by the vinegar stimulate C, sufficiently to discharge it, so long as that road is open, more quickly than they stimulate D. That C should be dischargeable only so long as the foot is free implies that the activity of the neurone is in some way conditioned by its relation with the muscles which it innervates. When the foot is held this relation is interfered with, giving to the impulses generated by the continued action of the vinegar time to overcome the resistance of D. The simile of the opening up of paths is fairly applicable to the results which follow the use of artificial stimuli. Neurones seem to link up in series under the influence of the impulses which bombard them, popping like fireworks united by a common fuse. Experimental evidence points to the following conclusions : (1) Resistance is offered at a synapse. This resistance must be overcome before an impulse can get through from neurone 1 to neurone 2. (2) The impulse does not, properly speaking, pass from 1, through 2. It infects 2, causing it to discharge a fresh impulse. (3) Time is of the essence of this process. Hither the impulses head up at the synapse, or, passing through into the neurone, they produce a cumulative effect within it, which provokes it to discharge. (The latter hypothesis, which is the less likely of the two, transfers the resistance from the synapse to the neurone to be infected.) These conclusions are based upon experiments of the following kind: The minimal stimulus which will evoke a reflex action is determined. A stronger stimulus is then applied. The reflex occurs more promptly, and is more pronounced. But on further increasing the stimulus, it is found that the limit of effectiveness is soon reached. The proportional relation of response to stimulus is much less evident than it is when the experiment is tried with a nerve-muscle. Choosing a reflex action easily pro- voked, the afferent path is stimulated with an electric current interrupted fifty times a second. The impulses which flow down the efferent path to the muscle follow one another at the rate of about ten a second. A column of nerve-fibres within the spinal cord is stimulated fifty times a second. Again, the discharge into anterior roots has the natural rhythm of about ten. The cortex of the “motor area” of the great brain is 310 THE BODY AT WORK stimulated with a rapidly interrupted current. The muscles _ which it governs contract with their natural rhythm. The — cortex is sliced away, and the stimulus applied to the white matter beneath. A similar result is obtained. Evidence such as this points to an independence of action on the part of the neurones which one can express only in terms of re- sistance and explosion. But there is another line of thought which leads to the development of a picture of the working ner- vous system which seems at first sight incompatible with the one that we have sketched. The phenomenon of the knee-jerk (p. 274) reveals a nervous system so intimately linked together, so homogeneous, so mobile, that no event, however trivial, occurs in any part without sending a vibration throughout the rest. Instead of a multitude of batteries enveloped in a labyrinth of wires interrupted by myriads of switches which are crackling on and off, the image of a sheet of water better figures our conception—a material so frictionless that it is a-ripple from side to side and end to end, from the most distant rivulet which feeds it to the farthest trickle in which it drains away. It is a fluid in a state of infinite commotion, the movements of its particles varying in amplitude from tremulous quiverings which scarcely frost the silver of its surface to waves which, breaking on the muscular system, throw it up in heaps. The vinegar experiment seems to demand a scheme of batteries and wires. The knee-jerk points to a continuous conducting medium. Other phenomena suggest the superposition of the two pictures ; the conception of a nervous system consisting of a uniform medium conducting, not indifferently in all directions, but with such freedom that from our point of view the paths are infinite in number ; and within this conducting medium nerve-cell bodies and their processes which collect and distribute groups of vibrations sufficiently strong in com- bination to produce visible effects. In order that one of these neurones may be stimulated to discharging-point, the medium by which it is surrounded must be thrown into such a state of agitation as suffices to infect it. The considerations which point to the formulation of this double or superposed scheme are such as follow: The passage of tone-impulses does not appear compatible with the ideas we have formed on other evidence of synaptic resistance and neuronic discharge. They are too — feek sble for nab: a mechanism. The short “reflex time” of a en knee-jerk points to the passage of the agitation up a sensory root to the spinal cord, and through a non-resistant medium to the environment of the motor cells which it dis- charges, missing the neurone or neurones which intervene in the case of ordinary reflex actions. This is an illustration of the way in which tone-impulses, which we imagine as con- ducted by the non-resistant medium, pass over into discharges which produce visible effects. Again, the phenomena of in- hibition appear to require the supposition of extra-neuronic conduction. Whenever a reflex path is in use, all other paths in its neighbourhood are closed. The passage of impulses leading to a particular reflex action is favoured by the suppression of © conduction in its vicinity. When A is talking to D through the nerve-telephone, B and C are compelled to hold their peace. Inhibition is a phenomenon of universal occurrence. In re- lation to various actions, it is sufficiently pronounced to be visible in the effects which it produces. A simple experiment will illustrate this. Holding water in the mouth has no effect upon respiration, but during the act of swallowing respiratory movements are suspended. Whilst the swallowing reflex is occurring the respiratory reflex is inhibited. This might be attributed to the volitional control of respiration, and cer- tainly when attention is being directed to the process volition plays a large part. But if a finger is placed on the pulse, it is possible to detect that, during the act of swallowing, the pulse quickens, owing to the suppression of the slowing action of the vagus upon the heart. Here is a case in which inhibition is in no degree a voluntary action. Nor is it of any value as an adjunct to the particular reflex with which it is associated. It is an illustration of the universal rule that activity of any one spot in the nervous system is the cause of the quieting of the surrounding area. Impulses which reflexly check the heart cannot get through the medulla oblongata whilst the swallowing impulses are traversing it. Inhibition has been described as a drainage of nerve-force into the active area. On the structural side it seems to require the conception of an extra-neuronic substance which, agitated in the vicinity of the cells which are to be discharged, is brought to rest around neighbouring cells. The promulgation through the nervous system of the state which, when it reaches the centres of consciousness, produces pain a seems to call for an hypothesis of extra-neuronic conduction. 4 Any reference to pain in a work on physiology needs a few words of preface, since popularly the term “pain” is used in various senses. When I see pink geranium and nasturtiums growing in the same flower-bed, I may exclaim: “ It is positively painful.” The want of harmony, and at the same time the insufficiency of contrast, of chalky pink and translucent orange, jars my esthetic sense. Dislikes, however well founded, are ruled out in thinking of the physiology of pain. Further, in defining pain, we must be careful to isolate the real thing, and _ not to confuse it with sensations which seem to lead up to it. If, putting my finger in a pair of pincers, I touch it as lightly as possible, the first sensation is one of contact ; a little harder, and it becomes a sense of pressure ; harder still, and all sense of contact or pressure is lost in pain. It is usual to regard pain as sensation carried to excess. But neither is this physio- logical. An excessively bright light or an excessively loud sound is disagreeable. It causes a sudden movement for the purpose of avoiding it—just such a movement as one would make if one touched a red-hot poker—but it is not, strictly speaking, painful. Not uncommonly in cases of accident or disease of the spinal cord a sharp distinction is drawn between the sense of touch and the capacity for experiencing pain. Below the injury the patient retains his sense of touch un- diminished in acuteness, but no blow, or cut, or burn, causes him any pain. The pain caused by squeezing the finger in a pair of pincers is not, therefore, an excess of touch sensation. Pain begins to be experienced in the skin just when the object applied to it is affecting it to an extent which might do harm. If the point of a needle touches it, it causes pain as soon as the pressure is a trifle less than that needed to pierce its surface. A hot object begins to hurt when the temperature reaches 48° C. —almost enough to coagulate the tissue fluids. Pain is not a discriminative sensation. If I hold my arm out at right angles, I am conscious for the first few minutes of its weight, and have, besides, some sense of the traction exerted by the muscle of the shoulder. At the end of ten minutes these sensations are merged in pain, and for some time after lowering the arm the shoulder-muscle aches, much as it does in rheu- ‘THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 313 matism. Pain is an effect upon consciousness, which absorbs, engulfs, and therefore obliterates sensation. To use an ancient phrase, “It is less that I feel pain than that I am pain.” If we speak of the capacity for pain as a sense, we may call it for the purpose of our present argument the “sense of damage.” The nerves of the skin are acutely affected by any agent which is likely todo harm. It is their business to convey to the central nervous system an influence which so affects it as to set up in consciousness the condition of pain. Sensations of damage evoke reflex movements by means of which the part of the body likely to be injured, or the whole body, is removed to a safe distance. It being the duty of the skin to give this warning, a service of nerves sensitive to noxious agents has been developed which scouts in co-operation with the services devoted to the recognition of physical contact and heat and cold (cf. p. 425). If, imagining that the fire has not been lighted, I touch an almost red-hot stove, I acquire quite a considerable amount of information of which I am able to make use. I gain an accurate notion of the situation of the stove, and I put the right part of my finger in my mouth. The skin sends to the brain the ordinary sensations of touch and pressure before the condition of pain is established. In seeking for a definition of pain, we must eliminate the two attributes which have characterized all the forms of stimulation which we have considered up to the present time: (1) The tendency to pro- voke movement; (2) the supply of information. If I am suffering from a whitlow, the last thing that I am disposed to do is to jerk my finger about. Although it enhances the urgency of skin-reflexes, pain, in general, inhibits movement instead of provoking it. This is well illustrated in pleurisy. So long as a man is healthy he is quite unconscious of the fact that at each respiration the lower part of the lung slides on the lining of the chest-wall ; but commencing inflammation on the surface of one of the lungs causes intense susceptibility to friction, and the pain produces an effect which the man is quite unable to produce by an effort of will; it stops the movements of the chest on the damaged side. Pain is in- hibitory, not stimulant. It is not, properly speaking, a sen- sation. Frequently being mixed with sensational elements, it conveys topographical information ; but pure pain approaches rr SPARE eC en ene Nee ee rs 314 THE BODY AT WORK _ in quality the nebulous sense of distress of a patient who, when asked where he felt it, replied : ‘‘ Nowhere ; but there is a deal of it in the room.” Sufferers describe pain in figurative language, as ‘‘ burning,”’ “stabbing,” ‘‘ throbbing,” ‘“‘aching,’ and so forth. Two persons afflicted with the same lesion, the same source of pain, use approximately the same terms. Hence we cannot say that pains do not differ in character. But this is not a sufficient reason for assigning any specific quality to pain. It varies in severity, in continuity or intermittence, in sudden- ness of onset, in the sensations which accompany it, in the emotional tone to which the disturbance of the organ from which it proceeds gives rise, in the tenseness of the part affected and its consequent sensitiveness to a throbbing pulse. All these things make a complex of pain plus sensation, which causes toothache to differ from headache, and both from the pain of burned skin. But they do not give specific qualities to different varieties of pain. This being the case, there is no need to presume the existence of special nerve-endings for the reception of pain, or of a special region of the cortex of the brain for its reception. On the contrary, the evidence is conclusive that the nerve-fibres which serve the more highly specialized senses, which have well-defined connections in the cortex of the brain, do not convey the influence which enters consciousness as pain. It is the innumerable nerves which have no specialized receptors that take up pain. The afferent nerves of the viscera—the vagus and sympathetic—convey no impulses which enter consciousness, so long as the tissues which they supply are healthy. They have no representation in the cortex. The organs with which they are connected (with trivial exceptions, easily accounted for) are absolutely insensitive to injury. Before the virtues of chloroform were known—in the days when, however severe the operation, the patient had to nerve himself to bear it without an anesthetic—surgeons proved that the liver or the intestines, or practically any other viscus, may be cut or cauterized without the patient being aware that it is being touched. The same is equally true of the brain itself. But if damage in a viscus is set up gradually, its nerves convey to the central system an agitation which has the most pronounced results upon consciousness, and on the way ‘THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 315 profoundly affects the reflex actions which the spinal cord can carry out, and also its capacity as a conductor. Once in his life, perhaps, a man passes a gall-stone ; for generations such a thing may not have happened in his family. Yet the man finds that he is provided with a nervous apparatus which conveys to consciousness intensest pain. It is difficult to think of pain as travelling along nerves in the form of rhythmic impulses, similar to those which produce in consciousness the effects which we have distinguished as sensations. A few lines above we stated that no impulses which affect consciousness normally travel up the vagus or the sympa- thetic nerve, limiting the term “impulse,” perhaps unjustifi- ably. The vagus conveys an influence which enters our experience, as hunger. Probably other states of feeling for which we have no names, which resemble pain and hunger and. their opposites, are set up through the agency of visceral nerves. Fifty years ago attention was called to the difficulty of finding pain-paths amongst the white tracts (nerve-fibres) of the spinal cord. It is as difficult to point them out now as it was then ; but the inference that pain travels up the grey matter has given way to the ‘neurone theory”; under a misapprehension as the writer holds. Pain travels slowly. If one happens to notice a person who unsuspiciously touches a hot surface, one observes that an interval elapses between contact of his finger with the iron and the exclamation with which he “ relieves his feelings.”” It amounts to more than a second—if the iron is not very hot, to several seconds—whereas the “‘ reaction time ” for touch is only one seventh of a second. The slowness of movement of pain through the nervous system can on the neurone theory be explained only on the hypothesis that it travels from link to link along a very long chain of very short neurones. That pain is a state of the grey matter rather than a succession of impulses, and that (within the cerebro-spinal axis) the state is transmitted through an extra- neuronic medium, seems a simpler explanation. The state set up in the segment of the cord in which afferent fibres, conveying pain from viscera, embouch affects its con- ductivity. It subdues reflex action through the segment, and at the same time facilitates or reinforces the transmission of Sa, Vas ta wees FT Sa Ne i par Sant, = TAS oe ? Line OPS 316 THE BODY AT WORK sensory impulses towards the seat of consciousness. This shows itself in the apparent increased sensitiveness of the skin of the area of the surface supplied by the posterior root which joins the segment of the spinal cord into which the pain in- fluence is also being poured. For example, afferent sympa- thetic nerves from the cardiac end of the stomach join the sixth and seventh thoracic spinal nerves. Other afferent fibres run up the vagus to the medulla oblongata. When the cardiac end of the stomach is diseased, pain is referred to the skin area supplied by the sixth and seventh dorsal roots. The ordinary inevitable stimuli acting upon this area cause pain. Experi- mental stimuli which elsewhere would be felt as touch or warmth are painful. The impulses to which they give rise pass through pain-agitated segments of the spinal cord. The vagus nerve carries its pain influence to the medulla oblongata. Now, it happens that the sensory nerve of the face—the fifth— spreads for a considerable distance up and down the axis of the brain. The fifth nerve in consequence pours its sensory impulses into a region which is pain-agitated by those fibres of the vagus which come from the cardiac end of the stomach. Hence disease of that organ gives rise also to an “illusion ”’ of pain—pains and illusions of pain are philosophically indis- tinguishable—on the surface of the head. The viscera, having no direct access to consciousness, appear by deputy. When the stomach is distressed, it makes its appeal to the whole body politic for considerate treatment through certain nerves which have the privilege of appearing at Court. The message is mis- read as coming from the front of the chest—“‘ heart-burn ”— or from the shoulder, or from the scalp, or from the other skin areas which these nerves serve. When the liver is in trouble, consciousness, having no knowledge of its where- abouts—is it the business of hand and eye to explore another man’s liver, or incumbent upon the mind to accept their findings ?—infers that the cry comes from the shoulder. Nor have the tissues beneath the root of the nail, or the muscle of the shoulder, or the pulp of a tooth, any direct representa- tion in consciousness ; but since the pain-condition in the grey matter converts it into a microphone, messages from neighbouring structures which otherwise would fail to arouse attention, after traversing the pain-segments of the nervous THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 317 system, ring out clearly, and hence the mind locates approxi- mately the “pain” of the whitlow, the muscle-ache, the decayed tooth. Sufferers from toothache are familiar with . the phenomenon of the spread of pain from a definite spot to the whole jaw or the whole side of the head, dependent upon the spread of the pain-agitation from the segment of the axis of the brain in which the dental nerve ends to neighbour- ing segments. Our ability or inability to localize a pain does not depend upon the presence or absence of pain- nerves, but upon the existence or non-existence of nerves coming from the same organ, or from its neighbourhood, and capable of conveying impulses to the seat of consciousness. In passing through the part of the spinal cord or of the axis of the brain which is disturbed by the influence exercised by a damaged organ, silent impulses acquire force sufficient to render them audible, and combine with the pain to produce a feeling which consciousness can analyse, to a certain extent. Informed as to its whereabouts by these accentuated sensations, consciousness recognizes a sense of pain limited in its topo- graphical extension. | Sneezing when a bright light falls upon the eye is a curious illustration of the exaggeration of the effectiveness of sensory impulses when they happen to be poured into an agitated segment of grey matter. About one person in every three is affected in this way. A friend of the writer, who was par- ticularly sensitive, rising in the night because he heard his child cry, three times lighted a candle and three times sneezed it out before he could watch the application of match to wick without suffering from a nerve-storm. Some nervous dogs— especially fox-terriers—are very liable to this neurosis. Many persons who do not sneeze feel, when the sunshine stimulates their retinee, a tickling in the nose. Again the illusion is to be traced to the door of the fifth nerve—the sensory nerve of the whole of the face. The nose is the true tip of the body. Morphologically it is anterior to the eyes. Just as the fifth nerve extends its distribution to the nose, so also its root- fibres extend their connection within the axis of the brain forwards, until they traverse the mid-brain, the primary centre of the optic nerve. A bright light, by stimulating the optic nerve, sets up a commotion in the mid-brain. The ordinary 318 | THE BODY AT WORK every-moment impulses from the nose, carried by the fifth nerve to this region, ought not to appear in consciousness at all; but owing to the excited condition in which they find the grey matter they assume an importance which does not belong to them, and discharge the reflex action of sneezing, just as they would do had one taken snuff. Several lessons are to be learned from this phenomenon—as, for example, one which cannot be too often impressed, that the impulses which appear in consciousness (or, more accurately, the impulses to which attention is directed) are but a most insignificant fraction of those delivered by sense-organs to the central nervous system. The impulses which give rise to the sensation of tickling in the nose are not exceptional impulses which happened to be started when the light fell on the eye. They were reaching the brain in a steady flow before the agitation of the mid-brain gave to them exceptional force. No consideration regarding the working of the nervous system has a more important bearing than this. We cannot picture to ourselves the activity of the sensory nervous system. Our experience is limited to the scattered sensations which we perceive. Are the sensory nerve-endings incessantly responding to external forces, throwing an almost continuous procession of impulses up each of the millions of nerve-fibres which connect them with the central system? Such a conception is probably nearer to the truth than the conception which we should develop if we trusted to experience. Yet even experience tells us that an infinity of messages is delivered to the brain, of which consciousness takes no account. Changing trains at a roadside station in France, my attention was attracted by an electric bell on the platform, which was ringing con- tinuously. ‘“‘ Why does the bell ring ?”’ I asked the station- master. “To make known that everything goes well,” was the response. “If it stops, something is wrong.” ‘“‘ But do you not become so accustomed to it that you cease to hear it ?” “Yes, truly ; it rings day and night. One does not pay atten- tion to it until it has stopped.”” Sensory impulses generated by the contact of my skin with the chair that I am sitting on are incessantly ringing the bell of consciousness. I should notice them immediately if they stopped. As it is, they do not attract my attention until they ring a little louder than usual, or until 4 ey — *f te ieee wa CL wie Bee naa Se As Veo TPs) crac, Whe = ee Soe eC ee ee ow at a 5 ¢ See a oe Dal ; - ‘THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 319 some particular group, owing to unrelieved pressure, produces a cumulative effect. Another lesson ; that the condition of the nervous system, and therefore its conductivity, is determined at any given moment by the sensory impulses which are reach- ing it. We cannot describe the effect of a bright light as pain, yet it agitates the grey matter, altering its state, in the same way as the nerve-inflow which we recognize as pain. A wet rag on the forehead does not assuage a headache by cooling the brain (cf. p. 106). The headache is “in the scalp.”’ The cool wet rag diminishes the dilation of the bloodvessels of the fore- head, and quiets the impulses from the skin which are pouring into a tract of grey matter pain-agitated by the influences ascending a visceral nerve—usually the vagus. _ It is necessary to warn the reader that a reversion to the old idea of “conduction through grey matter ’’—.e., otherwise than by a chain of neurones—is unorthodox. It is set forth here because it seems to the writer that the various phenomena which have to be accounted for fit in best with the hypothesis of a double path. If evidence of the anatomical possibility of extra-neuronic conduction is asked for, it may be pointed out that the chrome-silver and methylene-blue methods, upon which our knowledge of neurones is based, do not, in the very nature of the case, show that grey matter consists only of neurones and their obvious branches. As they select par- ticular elements of structure, we can never by their use alone know what they fail to show. Attention may also be called to the fact that the same staining process which reveals peri- cellular nets (p. 301) shows also a structure resembling a net- work in the substance which intervenes between them. Truly the method is a rough one. It may well be thought that the nitric acid used to fix the tissue may cause strange coagula- tions with solution of uncoagulated substance ; but, as was re- marked with regard to the pericellular nets, regular patterns indicate architectural differentiation. But whether these nets do or do not give hints as to the nature of the conducting medium, there is no difficulty in finding sufficient material, after all the substance entering into the formation of the conducting neurones, as we imagine them, has been accounted for. Hz hypothesi, the conducting material is provided by the fibrils of the sensory nerves in their extensions beyond the limits to poe ee he a Ae Sa pe Te ae eT A ee Sey ar Ns ee SS eh Fre ae “5390 MEE BODY Mo WORR tee which the deposit of subchromate of silver extends, when the F _ chrome-silver method of displaying neurones has been used. Sensation-impulses enter neuronic chains. The condition which, when it affects the seat of consciousness, is known as pain, progresses up the vertebrate neuropil. Energy is developed within the nervous system. The force of impulses is adjusted to the resistance which they have to overcome. Stimulation of the millions of twigs of the vagus nerve in the lungs brings about the gentle move- ments of ribs and diaphragm which constitute peaceful respira- tion. A crumb of bread touching the mucous membrane of the larynx stimulates a few of the endings of the same vagus nerve. Like an avalanche, the impulses gather head as they advance, causing, not the diaphragm and intercostal muscles alone to do their utmost, but calling into action half a dozen accessory muscles of respiration. It is difficult to account for this reverberation of the messages which clamour for the ejec- tion of the crumb of bread without figuring them as spreading from neurone to neurone, urging each in turn to deliver its maximal discharge. Neurones are provided with material which serves as a store of energy. In their cell-bodies, including their dendrites, are to be seen coarse granules of nucleo-protein, which, being fitted in between groups of neuro-fibrille, assume an angular form. They are known as Nissl’s corpuscles, or are termed ‘* tigroids,”’ owing to the spotted appearance which they give to the substance of a cell. If the nerve-cells of birds be ex- amined just after they have alighted from a migratory flight, the granules are found to be few and small. In a bee re- turning to the hive at evening with its last load of pollen, they are smaller than they were when it commenced its morning’s work. They disappear in certain pathological conditions, and under the influence of various drugs ; and since their presence is revealed by staining, their disappearance is spoken of as ‘* chromatolysis.”’ The wasting of tigroids during functional activity proves clearly that nerve-cells do work, in the physical sense. Energy is expended in transmitting messages from receptor to effector, from sensory cell to muscles, from recipient nerve-ending to glands. Have nerve-cells any privileges or duties? Their re oo oa a sl aD a m4 4 Tor av? << x = > sar y ‘THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 32 eg <, ae : ; functions, so far as we have considered them hitherto, are tion and connections determine the direction in which they conduct, and the degree in which they reinforce stimuli im- pressed upon the nervous system by the environment, including what may be termed the internal environment, food in the alimentary canal, secretions in ducts, and so forth. Have the { ' Fig. 22.—THE BoDY OF A MOTOR NEURONE. In its centre is a large clear spherical nucleus, with a nucleolus. The body-substance is pro- longed into five dendrites and an axon. Neuro-fibrille are seen in dendrites and axon. They traverse the body of the cell in all directions, in little bundles which are separated by angular granules of stainable substance (tigroids). cells any directive or executive functions? There is no evidence that they have ; nor, it must be added, is there any line of reasoning which leads inevitably to the conclusion that they have not. Remembering that, until recently, it was the custom to solve all obscure problems and to shelve all diffi- culties by conferring human attributes upon nerve-cells and collections of nerve-cells, termed “centres,” a physiologist admits the negative with reluctance. The unconscious argu- 21 automatic, from a mechanician’s point of view. Their situa- > i ea iS i eS * ment in ‘he past used to run somewhat thus : “I decide to act a or to abstain from action. The nerve-cell is the mechanism by : means of which I decide. Therefore the nerve-cell decides.’ (In the past a distinction was drawn between the cell-body and its processes, but that, we now see, was absurd.) It is very difficult to relinquish completely this attitude of mind. I feel, I remember, I will. There must be a something which feels, remembers, wills. But a physiologist finds in the nervous system no evidence of a capacity for any function other than that of conduction, with adjustment of the force of current. He can no more discover feeling, memory, or will in a chain of neurones than he can find music in a violin. He hears the strings singing in the breeze. He can twang them with an electric shock. But he has no vision of ghostly performers, no glimpse of the conductor’s baton. Yet he knows, as every sane man knows, that the neurones are the instruments played in the orchestra of mind. He knows that, while all are sound- ing, some are muted, in order that the others may produce a dominant effect. He knows, too, whenever he decides to con- tinue writing or to close his notebook, that the conductor is raising the baton or allowing it to sink by his side. A neurone or nerve-cell is a transmitting link. It is scarce a thing to wonder at that physiologists, having wrestled successfully with the superstition of the “ pontifical nerve- cell,” are unwilling to reinstate it even as doorkeeper in a free church. It may be that it exercises some discretion in ad- mitting impulses, but until its authority as a guardian of the path which stretches behind it has been established, it is better to regard it merely as a door which swings open whenever pressed with sufficient force. Is it possible to classify neurones according to their function ? They can be classified according to size, and, with some degree of completeness, according to form. But if, as we believe to be the case, size and form are governed by purely physical re- quirements, the divisions into which the cells fall have no physiological significance. The motor cells of the spinal cord and axis of the brain are large and irregular in shape. Their dimensions are clearly dependent upon the size, thickness rather than length, of the nerve-fibres which are drawn out from them. They discharge impulses to groups of voluntary THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 323 muscle-fibres at a considerable distance. Small cells could not do the work. Precisely similar reasons can be given for the large size of the cells of Purkinje in the cerebellum, which transmit the elaborated product, as we may term it, of this organ to the great brain ; and for the dimensions of the large pyramids of the great brain, which convey its decisions to the spinal cord. The small pyramids of the cortex of the great brain distribute the first crude impressions of sensations to neighbouring (association) areas of the cortex. A cell of Purkinje (Fig. 23) has a more complicated, and at the same time @ more regular, form than any other nerve-cell. It resembles an exceedingly richly branched espalier pear-tree, set at right angles to the narrow convolutions of the cerebellum ; a dispo- sition easily accounted for, when the structure of the cortex of this organ is considered. Its outer layer in which the espalier processes ramify is traversed longitudinally by an infinity of nerve-threads, the bifurcated axons of granules. These granules are small neurones which take up impulses from afferent (“mossy ”’) fibres, and distribute them to the dendrites of the Purkinje cells—each collecting from a few fibrils only of the sensory channels. (The word “sensory” is used to indicate that sense-organs are their provenance, and not that their messages become sensations.) The numerous spreading branches of a Purkinje cell, disposed in a transverse plane, are obviously arranged to hold up and keep apart these myriads of longitudinal threads. A cerebral pyramid is shaped like a fir-tree. It is placed in a definitely stratified layer. By its branches it collects impulses from the superficial strata, which it transmits through its stem to the white matter beneath the cortex. The various parts of the central nervous system have work of different kinds to do, and we find interposed in the circuits which compose the several parts cells of various types. We speak of the large cells as “ motor,” the granules as “‘ sen- sory,” the small pyramids as “ association ” cells—such terms indicating the positions which they occupy in the ares, but not defining their functions. Of specialization of function the physiologist cannot obtain a hint. He cannot classify nerve-cells in groups concerned in reflex action, in feeling, in remembering, in willing, in thought. On the contrary, he can assert with confidence that such distinctions are not to be drawn. 21—2 type of cell is found for which, in the present Aste of stam certain af = it is impossible to account. We mention these cells lest it ~ should be inferred, from what has been said above, that all neurones can be fitted into a simple scheme of conducting ares. In the spinal ganglia there are neurones whose axons divide to form “baskets”? around other ganglion-cells. In the cere- bellum there are similar cells, the axons of which divide into branches, which break up to encase Purkinje cells. Cells of | the same kind are found in a few other situations. In some cases the end-branches which enter into the formation of the baskets are few in number, and thick and clumsy. They grasp the body of the cell which they surround, with gouty fingers, as it were. In other cases the basket is a tangle of fine threads. It is difficult to see what role cells of this kind can play in conduction. From the olfactory and optic centres nerve-fibres extend outwards to the olfactory bulb and retina. Here again is an arrangement which does not fit in with any scheme. We might multiply examples. But enough has been said, perhaps, to convey the impression which we wish to leave, that, although experiment abundantly proves that the nervous system consists of an association of sensori- motor conducting arcs, and although anatomical investigation demonstrates the existence of chains of neurones which take part in the formation of such arcs, it is impossible to reduce the system to schemata or to prepare diagrams in which all structural elements are, even hypothetically, fitted into place. It may be convenient at this point to call attention to the differences which distinguish the sympathetic system — the ganglia and nerves of the viscera and bloodvessels—from the system devoted to bringing sense-organs into connection with the skeletal musculature which we have chiefly considered hitherto. The fibres of the posterior root of a spinal nerve which convey impulses from the skin and muscular sense- organs, and the fibres of its anterior root which convey impulses to skeletal muscles, have a similar diameter of about 15 p. In addition to these, the roots contain fibres which carry im- pulses from and take them to the viscera. Those which bring impulses from the viscera vary greatly in thickness, some being as large as the other sensory nerves of the posterior P ferve. If all organs are removed from an animal’s chest and abdomen, a string of small pearl-like ganglia, united by a longitudinal cord, is seen lying on either side of the bodies of the vertebrz, one ganglion for each segment. This string of ganglia is termed the “sympathetic chain” (cf. p. 243). The small medullated fibres of the anterior spinal roots join these ganglia. Some of them arborize about their cells ; some pass by them to arborize in ganglia which lie farther afteld, on the course of the great bloodvessels and within the viscera. The axons of neu- rones whose cell-bodies are within a ganglion break up into bunches of non-medullated fibres. In this way the fibres of the sympathetic system are increased in number. Each of its neurones is a multiplying and distributing station. There is no evidence that it in any way serves as a “centre,” takes part in reflex action, or otherwise usurps the functions of the grey matter of the spinal cord. Nerve-cells are thickly strewn between the mucous membrane and the muscular coat, and again between the two layers of the muscular coat of the alimentary canal. It is not so certain that this system has no “ central ”’ functions. The remarkable degree in which the wall of the intestines re- tains its capacity for co-ordinated movement, after all nerves which reach it from the ganglia and through the vagus have been cut, suggests that the plexus of nerves within it does act to some extent as a reflex centre. If we leave the case of the _ intrinsic nervous system of the alimentary canal open, awaiting further proof, there is no reason for looking upon the sympa- thetic system as in any degree independent of the spinal cord and brain. It does its work on a large seale, and its work is of a low order. Nature does not need to connect up the viscera and bloodvessels with the central nervous system by means of fibres as thick as those used for skeletal muscles. It is more convenient to provide for the multiplication of the nerves— which must be extremely numerous, owing to the relatively minute size of the muscle-fibres for which they are destined— outside the central system than it would be to include the necessary distributive cells within it. Again, we find that a nerve-cell, when we see it at close quarters, shows no evidence 326 THE BODY AT WORK of administrative capacity. Although of a different shape, a ganglion-cell of the sympathetic system is as large and as complex in form and structure as a pyramidal cell of the cortex of the brain ; yet the work which it does is of a purely me- chanical order. It receives, reinforces, transmits impulses which reach it from the central nervous system. The often-repeated statement that a nerve-fibre is a drawn- — out process of a nerve cell-body has prepared the reader to anticipate that it dies when cut off from its central connection. When the axon is dead, the sheath which invests it rapidly loses its tubular character. If the situation of the cell-bodies of a nerve be known, it can be at once foretold on which side of the cut degeneration will occur. Suppose that the median nerve has been severed at the wrist. All nerve-fibres on the distal side of the wound must atrophy, whereas none of the fibres on the proximal side will be affected. The motor fibres have their cell-bodies in the spinal cord, the sensory in the spinal ganglia. Degenerations following lesions in the central ner- vous system have taught pathologists more about the course of the fibres in the white matter than any other class of observa- tions. Degeneration above the lesion is spoken of as ascend- ing, below as descending—not that it progresses upwards or downwards. It occurs throughout all the stretch of the fibre which has been isolated from its cell-body at the same time, or nearly so. The thought that impulses can no longer ascend or can no longer descend, as the case may be, has given sanction to the expressions “ ascending ”’ and “‘ descending ”’ degenera- tion. Restoration to functional activity of tracts of fibres which have degenerated in the brain or spinal cord never occurs, but severed peripheral nerves regenerate. Not that fibres join cut end to cut end, however clean the wound. A wound in the wrist which has divided the median nerve may heal in a few days “‘ by first intention,” so far as other tissues are con- cerned ; but the patient does not for two or three months recover the power of using the muscles of the hand which the nerve supplied or the sense of touch in the area of skin to which it was distributed. The ends of the axons on the proximal side of the wound have to grow downwards to establish new connections in the muscles and in the skin. Yo ee 2 The interval which elapses between the healing of the wound in the wrist and the restoration of sensation and power of movement is occupied in their downgrowth. The re-connection of regenerated nerves with their terminal apparatus presents to the mind a curious problem. There is no evidence that as function is re-established the brain has to re-learn the situation of the sensory spots on the skin, or to re-acquire skill in using the muscles which again come under its control. From the moment that the outgrowing nerves have recovered their terminal connections skin and muscles have their right representation in the brain, however much the two cut ends may have been twisted in their relation one to another. It seems inconceivable that each nerve-fibre can find its way to its original station ; but if it does not, our con- ception of the mode of working of the nervous system still needs much refining from the telephone-exchange analogy by which we naturally help out our explanations. If a telephone cable has been severed, it can be made useful again only in one of two ways. Either the two segments of every wire that has been cut must be reunited, or the subscribers’ numbers must be redistributed. The experiment of uniting the proximal segment of one nerve with the distal segment of another of a quite different function gives results which have an even more disconcerting effect upon our theory of the nervous system. The sympathetic cord of the neck and the vagus nerve lie very close together, alongside the carotid artery. The vagus is both afferent and efferent. The sympathetic is wholly efferent—+.e., it conducts impulses, which enter the sympathetic chain within the thorax, in the direction of the head. If both nerves are cut, and the end of the vagus turned round, so that itvis in apposition with the upper end of the sympathetic, its regenerating fibres make their way along the sympathetic cord, headwards, to the superior cervical ganglion. They arborize about the bodies of its ganglion- cells, just as the sympathetic fibres used to do. The vagus is a nerve of many functions. Amongst others, it inhibits the contraction of the heart, constricts the bronchi of the lungs, dilates the bloodvessels of the intestines, and helps in regulating the movements of these viscera. After it has taken the place of the upper segment of the sympathetic it dilates ; ou" o ahs : ae R - : + ise a ' CS ar oe Ress, 5 = 328 THE BODY AT WORK ES en the pupil, constricts the bloodvessels of the ear,erects the hairs of the head, as if to the manner born. To take another example, in a monkey the two nerves supplying respectively certain flexor and certain extensor muscles of the forearm were cut, and their ends crossed, so that flexor nerve-fibres grew down to extensor muscles, and extensor fibres to flexor muscles. There was no bungling of reflex actions or of voluntary actions when the new roads were first used. The monkey did not jerk its hand open when it tried to scratch or to grasp a nut. When experimental data first began to accumulate, physi- . ologists drew diagrams and made models of the nervous system in which they represented it as composed of conducting arcs. The arcs were superposed to indicate that they were of various grades—spinal for ordinary reflexes, bulbar for co-ordinated actions, through the grey matter in the centre of the great brain for “ ideo-motor ” actions, through the cortex of the great brain for voluntary acts. They spoke of authority and re- sponsibility, comparing the nervous system to an army or a club. It is premature to attempt a theory of the nervous system compatible with recent discoveries regarding its structure and mode of working, but it is clear that the diagrams and metaphors to which we have just referred were misleading. In place of attempting to disarticulate the machine, we ought to emphasize its structural unity. The results obtained by uniting heterologous nerves cannot be explained by reference to a model made of wires and pieces of cork. They do not fit in with any organization of human units or with any postal system or telephonic apparatus for transmitting news. Prob- ably the lines of thought which will prove most fruitful are somewhat as follows: (1) An efferent discharge occurs as the result of the opening of a circuit from a muscle back to the muscle. Afferent impulses—call them sensory, on the under- standing that this does not imply that they appear in con- sciousness—are ceaselessly flowing from receptors to effectors in the muscle. A sensation—in the case of skeletal muscles usually a skin sensation—reinforces them to discharging- point. If the spinal cord has been severed from the brain, the up-and-down flow does not reach beyond its grey matter. It is short-circuited. If the brain is in normal connection with the spinal cord, sensory impulses travel upwards to its cortex (with- hoe og po hy et) Bea i ae al ee Rh By oe THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 309 out, save in exceptional instances, arousing consciousness, or, as we should prefer to express it in this connection, without attracting attention) to a degree which varies with the several classes of receptor and with the animal. A monkey reduced to the condition of a “‘ spinal animal ”—i.e., with its spinal cord severed from its brain—is less competent than a dog, and a man is far less competent than a monkey. In other words, a man habitually uses his brain more than does a monkey, and a monkey more than a dog. The proportion which brain-weight bears to body-weight roughly indicates the part the brain plays in conducting the traffic of the body. (2) Communication within the nervous system is almost unrestricted. If, before the median nerve was divided at the wrist, receptor A usually initiated a current which passed through the circuit to effector X, and receptor B to effector Y, and if the new fibres which grew downwards lost their way so that the one which used to receive messages from A attached itself to B, and the one which used to transmit commands to X attached itself to Y, A is not thereby cut off from X, or B from Y. Such a mechanical association is restricted to our diagrams. It does not enter into Nature’s plan. The spinal cord is not scored with unchangeable paths. A messenger from A could always reach either X or Y. It was not the path, but the struggle with competing messengers, which directed him to X. When we endeavour to picture the mechanism of the nervous system, we find ourselves faced by phenomena which appear irreconcilable. One set of observations leads to the conception _of closed paths; another set points to an open conductor. The experimental crossing of nerves to which we have just alluded shows that the nervous system is adaptable, to a degree which seems extraordinary to anyone who attempts to compare it with any of Man’s devices for establishing communica- _ tion. Paths appear to make themselves. On the other hand, the more important, and therefore dominant, reflex actions, such as swallowing, breathing, the maintenance of position, are due to the union of receptors and effectors by lines which are either reserved for their sole use, or, if shared by other currents, it is on the understanding that they have a first and altogether prepotent claim. No competing impulses can divert them or block their way. All reflexes which in the 330 THE BODY AT WOR history of the race have established their right to dominance not only seize and hold a route through the nervous system, to the exclusion of all competitors, but, as we have already shown in the case of the swallowing impulse, the traffic in neighbouring routes is suspended for their benefit. At the other end of the scale we find reflexes which may be termed “ occasional,” in that, although of frequent occurrence, they exhibit illimitable variability in form. Occasional reflexes require, as a pre- liminary to their transmission, that the afferent impulses which give rise to them should secure for a time the exclusive use of the motor neurones by which they are carried out. The receptors bring the motor neurones into tune with themselves, ~ and while in tune they will respond to impulses from no others. But the tuning lasts for a short time only. Either receptor or neurone, or both, soon tire. There is no danger of a particular reflex being prolonged to the detriment of the organism asa whole. As an illustration of an occasional reflex, we may cite the scratching movement of a dog. Its skin is punctured by a flea. It scratches the place. A second flea bites it somewhere in the same neighbourhood. The dog does not shift its hind-foot so as to scratch midway between the two bites. It finishes out one scratch before paying atten- tion to its second tormentor. The exact position to which the hind-foot is raised depends upon the position of the irritant ; and since this may be shifted over a very considerable surface, the form of the reflex varies equally widely. Each of the very numerous receptors in the skin tunes a slightly different group of motor neurones ; and since a second irritant may reinforce the first, instead of making an alteration in the group of neurones which the reflex is discharging, it is clear that there is no fixed path uniting receptor A with neurones X, Y, Z and receptor B with neurones W, X, Y. If, however, the second irritation occurs at a spot lying at a considerable distance from A, in place of reinforcing the scratching movement which A has set going, it weakens and shortens it. The receptor C, which is calling for the discharge of a markedly different set of motor neurones, tends to inhibit those which are already active. These results are tested with precision upon a “spinal dog ”’ and with the aid of an electric needle, the other pole from the battery being a large flat plate placed in contact with the : THE NERVOUS SYSTEM | 331 animal’s body. The conception of definite paths, to which the contemplation of permanent reflexes gives rise, is in- _ appropriate to occasional reflexes. The latter show so wide a range of variability and adaptability as to prove that a given receptor may bring any of a great variety of groups of motor neurones into connection with itself ; just as a given group of neurones may be played upon by impulses from a great number of different receptors. We have called it a tuning of the motor neurones. One metaphor is as good as another. The physical process which in the brainless frog underlies the preparation for discharging motor neurones in the spinal cord, on the same side as the leg on which vinegar is placed, so long as that leg is free, and on the opposite side, when that leg is fixed, is unknown. We seem to catch a glimpse of a double- ness of action, receptors in the muscles combining with re- ceptors in the skin in determining the paths along which im- pulses shall be reflected—the efficient muscles sensitizing their own neurones to the tuning influence of impulses from sounding cutaneous nerve-endings. But it is impossible to formulate a working scheme in the present state of knowledge. Sense-Organs and Nerve-Centres.—A vast amount of labour has been devoted to the study of the external form of the central nervous system and to unravelling its internal structure ; to plotting out its various groups of nerve-cells, to disentangling its innumerable tracts of fibres. The surface of the brain and spinal cord has been mapped and measured. Every milli- metre of its substance has been cut into sections on the micro- tome. Organs which, fifty years ago, appeared too complicated for investigation have been described in the minutest detail. An immense accumulation of data is available for purposes of reference ; yet anyone who submits the theory of the nervous system as it is held at the present day to a general review must allow that the results of anatomical research enter but little into its construction. The reason for this is not far to seek. As knowledge has advanced, the apparent, or rather the ex- pected, complication of the system has given place to ideas of unity and simplicity. Its external configuration and the varied arrangement in “nuclei’’ of its nerve-cells may, without im- propriety, be described as accidental. The form of the body and the consequent location of the clients of the nervous 332 THE BODY AT WORK system determine the disposition and degree of concentration of its various business centres. It shows, when followed throughout the whole animal kingdom, extreme variability of its constituent organs, with absolute uniformity of plan. In- déed, from the physiological point of view the term “ organ ”’ is scarce admissible. It implies diversity of function in too high a degree. The several parts into which the central nervous system is obviously divisible co-operate so intimately as to preclude us from thinking of them as separate organs. If the citadel of the central nervous system is to be captured, all lines of approach must be tried. Its outward form must be studied, its minute structure examined with the micro- scope, its modifications in various animals compared, its de- velopment followed, its reactions to artificial stimuli tested, its pathological deficiencies and vagaries watched. Yet, of all the means which have been made use of in attempting to penetrate its secrets, the study of its history, by the methods of comparative anatomy and embryology, has probably con- tributed most to the development of sound ideas regarding the manner of its working. The first differentiation visible in the blastoderm—the globe of cells into which the ovum divides and out of which the embryo is built—has relation to the formation of the nervous system. If the earliest stages of its growth are followed, and the different phases through which it passes are compared with the forms which it assumes permanently in lower animals, the plan or type upon which it is constructed shows up distinctly. Looking down | the line to the earliest vertebrata, we can discern clearly the form of nervous system possessed by their prototype. Not that this “ideal ancestor ’’ ever existed. Experience teaches that it is unlikely that any animal that ever lived was abso- lutely regular and symmetrical in all its parts ; nevertheless, the type can be presented in a perfectly regular scheme. The ideal ancestor of the vertebrata was segmented, like a cater- pillar ora worm. Its mouth was not at the anterior extremity of the body, but two (or more) segments behind it. Every segment bore a sense-organ (at one period two sense-organs) on either side. Beneath each sense-organ there was a clump of “‘ orey matter.”’ Each segment also contained (although not at the earliest epoch) two clumps of nerve-cells and neuropil in § THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 333 a more central situation. These “ganglia” were united by _ longitudinal and transverse commissures. They received the axons of the cells which lay in the clumps beneath the sense- organs. They gave axons to various muscles. Such is the type out of which the modern nervous system has developed : two separate sense-organs and a complete nervous system for each segment, the sense-organs connected with the ganglion of the same side, the ganglia of the two sides bound together across the middle line, and each row of sense-organs and each row of ganglia united by longitudinal commissures into a chain. From the nervous system as we see it now the majority of these segmental sense-organs have disappeared ; but the mode of formation of the cerebro-spinal ganglia shows that they are the clumps of nerve-cells which lay beneath the vanished organs. In the nose and the eye the grey matter retains its original situation in the immediate vicinity of the receiving epithelial cells—as the olfactory bulb and the deeper (anterior) layers of the retina. The ganglia of the auditory nerve lie within the bones of the ear. Spinal ganglia are close to the spinal cord. Auditory and spinal ganglia contain only the cell-bodies of the first collecting neurones (sensory nerves) together with certain curious bracketing cells already referred to (p. 324), all the other constituents of the peripheral clumps of grey matter which are found in the olfactory bulb and retina having been withdrawn from the spinal ganglia into the axis of the brain and spinal cord. The sense-organs in front of the mouth have had from the beginning an immense advantage over the others as observing- stations. Whereas the body-organs collected information re- garding the things with which the animal came in contact, and consequently specialized in touch, pressure, temperature, and, in the case of fishes, sensitiveness to the chemical constitution of the medium in which the animal lived, the head-organs specialized in responsiveness to forces acting from a distance— particles suspended in air, vibrations of light, pulsations of sound. Sensitiveness to touch, if it is to be useful, must be widely distributed. The body-organs therefore broke into scattered groups of sense-cells. Touch-spots are scattered all over the surface, although they are set much closer together in the areas of skin which are usually the first to come into 334 depended upon their remaining compact. Progress in animal life, as we understand it—the rise from lower to higher forms —has depended upon increasing integration of the body and co-ordination of its functions. The nervous system is the agent which has accomplished this unification. Each step in © advance has depended upon the provision of more nerve- tissue for the lacing together of the various parts. We have seen already (p. 329) how intimate is the union of receptors and effectors of every kind via the spinal cord and brain. The overwhelming predominance in the direction of action of the nose, the eye, and the ear has led to the accumulation in their vicinity of the ever-increasing grey matter. The cerebral hemispheres, or “ great brain,’’ are pouched outgrowths from the first pair of ganglia directed towards the olfactory pits. The original eyes bore a similar relation to the second pair of ganglia—the epithet “ original ” implying that the eyes which we now use are not the organs with which our prevertebrate ancestors saw. First one of the original eyes disappeared, and then the other. The vestige of the second is still to be seen in the “pineal body ”’ which is found on the dorsal side of the brain of every vertebrate animal—in a mammal deeply hidden in the cleft between the cerebrum and cerebellum. In place of the pineal eyes two other sense-organs have specialized as eyes. They are constructed on a different plan, being, to put it shortly, pineal eyes turned inside out ; for whereas in the pineal eyes, as in most of the eyes of invertebrate animals, the rods and cones, which are the cells of the retina sensitive to light, are directed forwards towards the lens, the rods and cones of our permanent eyes are directed away from the source of light. This change has made it possible to provide more abundantly for their nutrition, and hence a greater power of discriminating separate points in space and of distinguishing colours is conferred upon them. The substitution of other sense-organs for the original eyes has complicated the pictures which are presented to us by a brain in its successive stages of growth ; but it does not pre- vent us from recognizing the general plan. Probably the secondary eyes, like their predecessors, belonged to a pre-oral segment. The sense-organs of a segment behind the mouth THE NERVOUS SYSTEM _ 335 developed into ears ; and the ear was in its earliest phases, and still is, something more than an organ of hearing. Its semi- circular canals give information of displacements in space. Knowledge of the position of its body is, to a fish, of far more importance than its ability to hear breakers on the rocks. Three - looped tunnels, opening at either end into a common chamber, are hollowed in the bone which contains the ear (cf. Fig. 38). Placed at right angles one to the other, they occupy all three dimensions of space. Open a notebook until, one of its covers lying horizontally, the other is vertical, and place a sheet of paper vertically against the bottom of the pages. A curved line drawn on each of these three surfaces will represent the three semicircular canals. Arrange another notebook in the same way, and let the two rest on the table with the two vertical covers inclining one to the other, anteriorly, at an angle of 90 degrees. The six surfaces will be in the planes of the six semicircular canals. Within each bony canal is a mem- branous. tube, to which nerves are distributed, filled with fluid. When the position of the head is changed, the fluid within the membranous tubes slides on their walls. It is left behind at the moment the movement commences. It over- takes its receptacle when the movement stops. The stimulus received by the nerve-endings is recognized as indicating an alteration in the orientation of the head. If the movement of the fluid is violent, as when one waltzes, the loss of the sense of position disconcerts the brain to such an extent that giddi- ness results. For a time the quiet assurance upon which so much depends, that one knows how the body stands in relation to its surroundings, gives way to a chaos of sensations. From the nature of the case, the information which the semicircular canals afford relates to change. They give no help in ascer- taining the position of the head when it is at rest. This must be the reason, although the connection is not very clear, for the waning of the effect in consciousness when stimulation is prolonged, and also for the very marked after-sensation. At the commencement of a voyage attention may be unpleasantly attracted to the rolling of the ship. After a few days it ceases to be noticeable ; yet when the voyager, the night after landing, wakes in the dark, he finds his bed-room as unsteady as his cabin. Rising hurriedly, the attempt to adjust his position to 336 THE BODY AT WORK the heaving floor (we speak from personal experience) may result in a heavy fall. Although this phenomenon must be classed — with other “ after-sensations,” it is so prolonged as to suggest that consciousness, having become accustomed to a world — which causes a backward and forward flow of endolymph, misinterprets the absence of sensation as indicative of change. Taste is, practically, a special kind of smell. A fish’s olfactory membrane, taste-buds, and chemical organs “of the lateral line”’ serve the same sense, although, no doubt, they are applicable to the analysis of different forms of matter in solution. Our ideal prevertebrate has now left its primitive undiffer- entiated condition. In front of its mouth it bears organs with which it searches the world. Close behind the mouth are its auditory and orienting organs. The rest of the surface of the body is endowed with the capacity of recognizing “taste,” temperature, and contact. Smell, sight, and orientation de- termine the development of the brain. The cerebrum which has eventually become, as the seat of consciousness, and hence the apparatus of mind, the dominant factor in the nervous system, was in the first instance the part of the brain concerned with the distribution to the muscles of impulses generated in olfactory organs. There is scarcely any indication in a fish’s brain of the representation in the cerebral hemispheres of any other sense, even that of vision. A bird’s brain presents a striking contrast to the brain of a fish. With the exception of the apteryx and other ground- birds of New Zealand, all birds are apparently destitute of the sense of smell. Vision is the sense upon which their activity depends. It has invaded the cerebrum, converting it into an organ in which sensations of sight are worked up into “ mind- stuff.” The optic lobe connection is restricted to the produc- tion of reflex actions in which vision is immediately followed by movement. All the senses are represented in the great brains of mammals. The cerebrum, which owes its existence to its connection with the favourably-situated sense-organ of the nose, and grew in importance when vision invaded it, has now taken in the senses of hearing, taste, and touch. Only what may be termed in general visceral sense, and the sense of orientation, are excluded. Looking back to the starting-point, we see a segmented io _ THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 337 - animal; its segments of equal value; its nervous reactions _ unisegmental, although linked in functional sequence. If it starts to walk, owing to stimulation of one of its sense-organs, the impulse to walk spreads from segment to segment. Com- paring the latest product of evolution with the earliest, we find that nervous tissue has concentrated at the anterior end of the body. The double chain of ganglia, now condensed into the axis of the brain and the spinal cord, still contain all the effector neurones by which muscles are called into action. Sensory nerves still arborize in the axis, providing the mechanism for actuating motor neurones. But the vast majority of intermediate or intercalated neurones have been attracted to the two huge brain-masses—the cerebellum and cerebrum. In the former all sensations (not conscious) con- nected with tone, position, orientation and equilibrium are worked into appropriate impulses for the regulation of the muscular system. In the latter all sensations which convey information regarding the relation of the environment, in- cluding the body, to the ego—the not-me to the me—are transformed into motor discharges which set a-going the move- ments (and the thoughts) by means of which the purposes of life are fulfilled ; for in the cortex of the great brain alone is the passage of nerve-currents accompanied by consciousness. Concentration of nerve-tissue allows of the combination of sensations. It also facilitates the no less important effect of mutual influence, interference. Sensations are suppressed, and therefore the multitude of reactions to which they would give rise are inhibited, in the interests of restricted and sustained movement or thought. The Cerebellum.—Sharks and other swift-swimming fishes have large, deeply fissured cerebella, for the cerebellum is the part of the brain which has gathered into itself most of the grey matter associated with balancing, attitude, posture. The cerebellum is in birds large and deeply folded. Developed from the ganglia to which the auditory nerve distributes im- pulses from the semicircular canals, it has established connec- tions with all the other nervous tissues concerned with sen- sations of position, strain, or pressure, including the eyes, which afford information regarding the position of our limbs _ felatively to the trunk, and of the whole body relatively to 22 Tet) Olea 338 external objects. Morphologically it is a median growth. — The adverb is one of those qualifying terms, convenient in — science, which direct thought without confining it. As used — above, it implies that anyone who passes before his mind the cerebella of all animals from fishes to Man, and in all stages of growth, from their earliest appearance in the embryo to their condition in the adult, sees the organ as a median promi- nence surmounting the medulla oblongata. The bulgings of its sides which, in human anatomy, are termed hemispheres, do not disturb its central, unpaired plan of structure. It has, it is true, a lateral appendage on either side (the combined flocculus and paraflocculus of mammalian anatomy), but this lobe, although of great historic interest, is so small, as com- — pared with the median growth, as not to affect our general conception of the form of the organ. By transverse fissures the cerebellum is divided into a series of lobes. In appearance the cerebellum varies greatly in the different classes and orders of Vertebrata. Yet underlying this variety there is marked unity of plan. A sagittal section of the organ of a shark, of a bird, of a kangaroo, of a dog, of a whale, of Man, shows that it is divided, from before backwards, into the same number of lobes in animals occupying every position from the bottom to the top of the vertebrate scale. A very little effort to grasp the significance of this mystic number, nine, convinces one of the hopelessness of any attempt to correlate the form of the cerebellum with the muscular development or sensory endowments of vertebrates as a sub-kingdom. It is the same for animals with limbs and animals without ; animals with well-developed noses or eyes, and animals destitute of one or other of these sense-organs. This uniformity is ex- tremely significant, when contrasted with the wide differences exhibited by the cerebral hemispheres. It shows that, unlike the great brain which mediates between the several senses and the muscular system, the little brain is concerned in bringing about adjustments to the environment which are equally im- portant to all animals, no matter how far they may depart from the common type. The cerebellum is crossed by deep fissures, dividing it into narrow convolutions or folia. The folia are grouped in nine lobes. If the reader has secured as an illustration the brain of a sheep, he will notice that the THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 339 lateral regions of the cerebellum present a complicated appear- ance owing to the contortion of the folia, which results from the unequal development on its sides of the several lobes. In its total size the cerebellum keeps step with the cerebrum, the right side of one organ being associated with the left side of the other. | A a rr jae is] oo co. A Sa “09 © co LAC ° c 26° o Vwi ® O70 00 oS ©, 00, Of 00 RL woos YF [2 ° ° ° ° NCOP ° © Fig. 23.—VERTICAL SECTION OF THE CORTEX OF THE CEREBELLUM, CUT PARALLEL WITH THE LONG AXIS OF A FOLIUM. A shows three cells of Purkinje, their espalier systems of dendrites being seen in profile. A ** mossy fibre ’’ enters the granular layer from the white matter. About a dozen of the granules are shown, each with four or five dendrites and a single axon. The axon bifur- cates in the molecular layer, its two branches running for a considerable distance to left and right along the folium. B shows the other nervous elements which are found in the cortex : a cell of Golgi with a ramified axon, a climbing fibre, a basket-cell, of which the axon divides into four branches, and a small stellate cell. The grey matter which covers the surface of the cerebellum, its cortex, is singularly regular in microscopic pattern (Fig. 23). It is divided into three sheets: superficially, the molecular layer in which the dendrites of the cells of Purkinje branch ; beneath this, the thin layer in which are situate the cell-bodies of these neurones ; thirdly, the layer of small cells, or granules. Cells of Purkinje and granules have been already described 22—2 340 (p. 303). To these must be added the stellate, bracketing cells — of the molecular layer, the axons of which divide to form baskets _ about a number of Purkinje-cells, and the cells of Golgi of the granular layer. These last are comparatively large cells, which have thornless dendrites, and axons which branch re- peatedly in the granular layer, without passing into the white matter which underlies the cortex. Two kinds of nerve-fibre bring impulses to the cortex : (1) “‘ Mossy ” fibres, which bear rosettes of filaments which distribute impulses to the granules ; and (2) “climbing ”’ fibres or “ tendril”’ fibres, which, passing through the granular layer, cling like ivy to the trunk and principal boughs of the dendritic processes of Purkinje-cells. The axons of the cells of Purkinje undoubtedly carry impulses away from the cortex, but their destination is not certainly known. The uniformity of structure of the cerebellum suggests that it “acts as a whole.” Anatomy gives no warrant for the expectation that work of different kinds is done by its several lobes. Its simplicity leads one to hope that its mechanism may some day be understood ; but at present there are so many gaps in our knowledge that it is difficult, perhaps hardly profit- able, to attempt to string together the few anatomical facts of which we are sure. By means of tracts of afferent fibres the cerebellum has a very extensive connection with the grey matter of the cerebro- spinal axis (including the optic thalamus) into which sensory impulses of all kinds are poured. Experimental results indi- cate that the organ distributes impulses to the whole length of the cerebro-spinal axis, from the level of the neurones which govern the muscles which move the eyes to its far hinder end. No nerve-roots enter it. Its afferent fibres are the axons of cell-bodies which lie in the posterior horns of the grey matter of the spinal cord and in the corresponding grey matter of the axis of the brain, especially that part related to the nerve from the semicircular canals. Another set of afferent fibres lies at the periphery of the spinal cord, forming one of the best defined of the spinal tracts. It is also one of the oldest, being found in the same situation in all vertebrate animals. Its fibres, which are exceptionally large, are the axons of cells which form a very definite column—the “ vesicular column of THE NERVOUS SYSTEM | 341 Clarke ”—on the median side of the posterior horn. Further than this we cannot go. We are ignorant of the nature of the sensory impressions collected by the cells of Clarke. The cerebellum also receives through its middle peduncle the axons of cells which lie in the pons Varolii on the opposite side ; which cells are discharged by impulses descending from the cortex of the great brain. It is not improbable that it gives to the great brain as many fibres as it receives from it. If we had no experimental evidence as to the part which the cerebellum plays in the harmonious working of the whole nervous system, we should infer from its structure and con- nections that it is somewhat mechanical, a co-ordinator of the activities of other parts rather than in itself a functionally independent organ. Pathological and physiological observa- tions very definitely justify this conclusion. They show that the cerebellum is not essential to life. It may be completely destroyed by disease or removed by operation without robbing the individual of any single function or capacity. Disease of the cerebellum does not diminish the patient’s sensitiveness to every kind of stimulus, nor does it deprive him of the use of any single muscle ; but it reduces him to the condition of a person who in gait, but not in mind, is habitually drunk. When he walks he staggers from side to side; when he stretches out his hand it trembles. His movements are jerky ; his head shakes, his eyes oscillate ; he suffers from a feeling of giddiness ; his speech comes haltingly. Cerebellar ataxia, which is a rare disease, resembles in many respects the much commoner “locomotor ataxia’? produced by disease of the spinal ganglia and the parts of the cord connected with pos- terior roots ; but careful analysis of the symptoms shows that they are due, not to want of the sensations which guide move- ments, but to inability to regulate the force of muscular con- tractions. A man suffering from locomotor ataxia falls when he closes his eyes, because, not being able to feel with his feet, he is dependent upon vision for information as to his attitude. When the cerebellum is diseased, the patient is no less unsteady with his eyes open than he is with them closed. The results of cerebellar disease or injury bring home to us the fact that a nice adjustment of movements is needed to main- tain equilibrium. A dog from which the cerebellum has been SD) Cee ee . 4 ; = tn 8S Fn - . > oa . — e* 342 THE BODY AT WORK removed retains all its natural enterprise, all its instincts, all — its emotions ; but every action which requires it to maintain its — centre of gravity in an unstable position gives it trouble. Placed in water so that its body is supported, it swims almost as well as a normal dog. It is, however, easy to lay too much stress upon the balancing function of the cerebellum. The disturbance of this function attracts our attention ; yet it is probably but the indirect result of the suppression of activities of a more widespread character. No animal ventures such liberties with its centre of gravity as the biped Man ac- complishes, without thinking, every time that he descends a flight of stairs. Yet the cerebellum of the limbless whale, that lives in a medium which decentralizes its gravity, so to speak, bears the same proportional relation to the rest of the nervous system as that of Man. Strangely enough, it is the only cerebellum in the animal kingdom which so closely re- sembles Man’s that it might be passed off as belonging to a human giant ; another reminder of the difficulty of deducing the functions of the several parts of the organ from a study of their relative development. What have a man and a whale in common which determines the identity in form of their cerebella ? How has it come about that two cerebra as widely unlike as a man’s and a whale’s should be associated with a common form of cerebellum ? If we apply to grey matter the distinction between sensory and motor nerve-tissue—having no exact terminology, it is difficult to avoid these metaphorical expressions—the cere- bellum is essentially a sensory development. It grows from the very margin of the infolding groove, which, when closed, becomes the central canal of the brain and spinal cord, its elements being marshalled in intimate association with sen- sory root-fibres. Its millions of loops formed by the axons of granules and the collecting processes of Purkinie-cells, are by-paths which tap the conductors of sensory impulses. From some—those, for example, which originate in the muscles and tendons, and in the semicircular canals—more of the impulse is diverted to the cerebellum, from others less. The organ has no motor functions. It does not discharge neurones which control skeletal muscles, or plain muscle, or glands. Yet it influences the passage of impulses through sensori- motor atisies, oat apparently its ey is universal. It F regulates tone, reflex action, voluntary action. There is no part of the nervous system over which its control is not felt. _ By its action on the apparatus which binds the infinity of - receptors which the body contains to its muscle-fibres and other effectors, it unifies the body. The cerebrum, as we shall see, is the organ which unifies the personality. In the progress of evolution two functions which were originally com- bined have, for convenience of concentration, been divorced. The great brain has been set free from the more mechanical part of the work. That it can perform the functions of the cerebellum as well as its own is proved in cases of congenital deficiency of that organ. In several instances malformation, amounting to a very considerable reduction in the size of the cerebellum, was not detected until after death, there being no symptoms of a sufficiently pronounced character to call attention to it during life. The Cerebrum.—aAll observations made on the great brain prior to 1870 showed it as absolutely inexcitable. Surgeons and physiologists agreed that cutting, burning, passing electric currents through its substance, neither yielded evidence of sensation nor movement of any part of the body. Concerning its structure little was known beyond the fact that whereas the grey matter, or cortex, which covers its surface contains nerve-cells, only fibres are to be found in the white matter which constitutes the greater part of its bulk. It seemed a hopeless task to attempt to make anything out of a mass of tissue so uniform in constitution and so irresponsive to experi- ment. Removing portions of it appeared to cause a general dulling of the intellect without loss of any particular mental quality. Physiologists, «therefore, spoke of the cerebrum as “functioning as a whole.” Phrenologists, having classified the various phases of mental activity as “faculties,” dis- covered “bumps” on the surface of the skull which they correlated with the possession of the several faculties in a marked degree. They parcelled out the brain in organs con- cerned with different kinds of thought ; but their localization of function was anatomically as baseless as their classification of the various aspects of mind, viewed as a system of philo- sophy, was absurd. In 1870 it was announced that electrical stimulation of certain areas of the cortex of the cereb of an animal under the influence of an anesthetic, and there- — : fore incapable of voluntary action, induces definite movements. _ Although the surgical applications of this discovery have q 4 proved immensely important, its physiological value, as afford- ing a method of investigating the functions of the brain, is ex- tremely small. Yet the discovery gave an impetus to the further study of the cortex, which has been rewarded with many exact results. By the discovery of its excitability to electric currents it was proved that the whole cortex has not exactly the same work to do, or—perhaps this is the safer form of statement—does not do its work in exactly the same way. As soon as it was known that it is divisible into areas differing in function, many methods by which the delimitation of the areas might be attempted were devised. The converging efforts made during the past forty years by comparative anatomists, histologists, physiologists, pathologists, and phy-— sicians, have resulted in the acquisition of an accurate, if very restricted, understanding of the construction and mode of working of the apparatus of thought. Of some of the new data the psychologist is able to make use; but so far as the physiologist is concerned, it is the vehicle of mind which is the subject of study, not its contents. A new subject has been created since 1870. There is there- fore nothing to be gained, so far as our present purpose is con- cerned, from the consideration of views which were current before that date; and since, as must always occur when a science is rapidly advancing, observations which logically should have been the first to be made were not thought of until it became necessary to devise methods of checking results obtained in other ways, we will consider the various sources of our information without regard to the chronological order in which they were opened up. The cerebral hemisphere contains two large central masses of grey matter, the nucleus caudatus and the nucleus lenticu- laris, often described as a single structure under the name “corpus striatum.” Their functions are unknown. The nerve-fibres which connect the cerebral hemispheres with the rest of the central nervous system form two thick limbs or crura on the under side of the brain. Each crus turns upwards a ‘THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 345 mat into its hemisphere, between the nucleus caudatus and optic thalamus (the latter belongs to the “‘ between-brain ”’) on the inner side, and the nucleus lenticularis on the outer. In this passage the compact crus, which is somewhat flattened, is termed the “internal capsule.” Immediately above the three grey masses the internal capsule disperses as a fountain of fibres which go to all parts of the cortex. Mingled with these radiating fibres are vast numbers of others, proper to the hemispheres, which run tangentially. Some, crossing the median plane, as the corpus callosum, bind the two hemi- spheres together. Others form tracts which can be followed from one end or pole of the hemisphere to the other. Groups of fibres, dipping but little below the cortex, unite nearly adjacent spots or neighbouring convolutions. The folding of the cortex beneath fissures is due to the necessity of disposing of a certain bulk of grey matter without increasing its thickness beyond the proper limit. Since the superficial area of a sphere varies as the square of its radius, whereas its capacity varies as the cube, it is possible for a fixed relation to be maintained between the amount of cortex and the amount of white matter in the brain, only by the folds increasing in depth as the size of the brain increases. Fissuring is a response to a mechanical need. This does not imply, how- ever, that the lines along which it takes place are devoid of morphological meaning. The similarity in pattern of the con- volutions and fissures in various animals, and the regular progress of their development in each individual, prove the contrary. If they are not absolutely trustworthy as boun- daries of areas of separate function—and further evidence will be needed before a decision can be pronounced upon this dis- puted question—they are in the main satisfactory as landmarks. As the nervous system grows, the axons of its neurones acquire their fatty (myelin) sheaths in the order in which they come into functional activity. The passage through them of impulses is the stimulus which leads to the deposition of fat. The study of the progress of myelination enabled the anatomist Flechsig to ascertain the situation within the brain of the tracts of fibres related to the several senses, and hence the traffic of the areas of the cortex to which they go. Glistening white streaks appear successively in the pulpy yellowish-pink < “> a 346 THE BODY AT WORK substance of the interior of the brain. At the time of birth all the fibres which enter or leave the cerebral hemispheres have acquired their myelin sheaths. In the baby’s brain the sense- organs have established all their connections with the cortex. No new fibres will appear in the nerves of the eye, the ear, or the other sense-organs, nor will their end-stations in the cortex be further multiplied. (The use of the expression “end- stations’ is legitimate so far as sensations are concerned ; notwithstanding that all sensory impulses are retransmitted by neurones in the cerebro-spinal axis.) But the cortex is very far from having finished its growth. It contains a large amount of embryonic tissue, which gradually spreads outwards from the developed areas into the surrounding unoccupied zones. The taking up of new territory, and the consequent increase in the size of the brain, is continued into adult life. The study of progressive myelination enabled Flechsig to divide the cortex into ‘‘sensory centres,” and intervening “association zones ”’; although, doubtless, the difference in function between the portions which receive sensations direct and the portions in which the products of sensation are worked up is one of degree, and not of kind. The structure of the cortex is not quite the same in sensory and association areas ; but it is everywhere so far from showing the diagrammatic simplicity which characterizes the cortex of the cerebellum as to make it difficult to summarize the modifi- cations which distinguish its various regions. To a considerable extent its elements shade one into the other, differing in size and in orientation rather than in form. Commonly it is de- scribed as divisible into five layers : (1) A thin superficial layer, containing cells of various forms and fibres derived from the cells of the deeper strata. Some of the cells are pluripolar, possessing several axons which run parallel with the surface. Their destination is unknown. They do not appear to form baskets like the cells of the molecular layer of the cerebellum. The dendrites of pyramidal cells extend into this layer. (2) The layer of small pyramids ; cells with a branching apical process, root-like dendrites from the basal angles of the pyramid, and an axon which sinks into the white matter. (3) Granules. Carmine or other nuclear stains show that small cells are present in very large numbers, especially in the sensory areas ; NERVOUS SYSTEM _ 347 Ai jini oy it Kiar Pia mater <== = | Molecular & =| ot aa Worigaytal | ee sa) Y + ghaaaaie Suocerficial | |) «:-l\°-\We . fibres er o WARK $ } peter. A 4 2 : | a IK a A . pyramids x j 4 | y\\\ OKs VAN Na iM we VANSIARS S\N) SATAN WW? . : @ Ai WENA Large stellate sV*. Was : Pik WA cells and. (7-4 PVA { E A LAS AA\ roan (a PEMA teers | ae & EIN Ral yA Broad mite e | 3 eX U) | O44) /4 wi Z WA tans | Bunn fibres h, iV OA : - (My NWAWKN aie VAG AN Oils ies , AN TATA\) Les Granules ' BS = on lope weilige SN Vee Y ALS Se os Large pyramids = Tangential, fibres 4) ay ‘ Polymorph at ih cells ' ‘4 A sf 4 Vy 4 | swe ‘ $ o ! NAS Afferent» \ \ Whi : sige el = fibres Rarroh a SE Fig. 24.—VERTICAL SECTIONS OF THE CORTEX OF THE CEREBRUM—A, OF THE VISUAL SENSORY ; B, OF THE VISUAL ASSOCIATION AREA. Between the two sections are shown the principal types of cell, at the levels at which they are severally found: a, small pyramid; 6, medium-sized pyramid; c, large pyramid. The size of a pyramid is an indication of the distance to which its axon extends before branch- ing ; the longer its traject, the more widespread, it would seem, is its terminal arborization. The axon of c, one of the very large pyramids found in this association area, passes to the front of the cerebrum, where it breaks up in an association area of the tactual sense of the hand, or of sensations concerned with the regulation of gait, or in a centre for movements of the eyeball. d, a tangential cell of the surface ; e, a Golgi cell with ramified axon ; f, a polymorph cell, with its axon directed towards the surface. In sensory areas, tangen- tial fibres and granules are more numerous; in association areas, small and medium-sized pyramids. Aa ae “ee eee ~ Get oe eo eS ~ OS SO cee | OB Oe Leth aS te he tee ee Oar Ne ae tae Re oe ~ : he p26 a ares ot ea Se a ee Pe ee eee we or x int : Soy € mB = eS r ed i E 348 THE BODY AT WORK ' but since they are not, like the granules of the cerebellum, coloured by the chrome-silver method, their form and the dispo- sition of their axons are unknown. (4) Large pyramids exactly similar in form to the small ones. Their apical processes are very thorny. Their axons give off several collaterals. Pyra- mids are the most conspicuous elements in the cortex. Properly speaking, they do not occur in layers, but are scattered throughout its whole thickness, although their cell-bodies- are not seen in either its most. superficial or its deepest strata. The largest are those of which the axons either descend into the spinal cord or pass to a very distant region of the cortex. They are found singly or in small clusters in the deeper levels. (5) Polymorphous cells, some of them pyramids lying on their sides, or even directing their axons towards the surface ; some fusiform or irregular cells; some Golgi-cells (p. 340). The axons of pyramids enter the white matter, and many fibres from the white matter radiate towards the surface be- tween the pyramids ; but the way in which afferent, sensory fibres are connected with the collecting processes, dendrites, of the pyramids is not known. We have already referred to thorns, and to the possible nerve-net (p. 301). Sheets of tangential fibres also occur in the cortex. A particularly distinct sheet divides the granules in the visual cortex into two strata. In sections of this region the sheet of fibres appears as a white line, distinctly visible without a lens. The limits of the several areas can be determined by ex- amining the structure of the cortex ; but the individual pecu- liarities of the various regions are not so marked as to indicate that they have different kinds of work to do; if by kinds of work we wish to imply that one part is ‘“‘ sensory,’ another ‘‘ motor,” a third concerned with “intellectual processes.” On the contrary, its relative uniformity shows unmistakably that all parts are engaged in the same work. Nevertheless, certain broad conclusions can be drawn with regard to the form of the neurones more immediately concerned with sensation, with motion—that is to say, with the discharge to the grey matter of the cerebro-spinal axis of the impulses which tall its neurones into activity—and with the secondary processes, called collectively “‘ association,’’ which occur within the cortex. Granules, as everywhere throughout the nervous system, are ct a A, re te Ln Ret gO PES, eee WD Eye Pte Se a oe er OL a Coat ar . S ? ss . ook vO pet a = L - pe THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 349 receivers and distributors of sensory impulses; although a study of the cerebral cortex does not justify the conclusion that they are necessary links in its sensori-motor arcs. Large pyramids are occupied with the nutrition of fibres which have a long traject through the system. Hence they are “‘ motor.” They constitute a marked feature of the area which is susceptible to stimulation. They occur also in the visual area and elsewhere. Small pyramids are associational ; that is to say, their axons do not leave the cerebral hemispheres. They distribute impulses from sensory areas to association zones, and from one part of an asso- ciation zone to another. The layer of polymorphous cells is relatively thicker in animals in which the cortex of the brain exercises less control over action than in animals in which the cortex is supreme—in a rabbit thicker than in a monkey ; in a monkey thicker than in Man. This layer is therefore said to be concerned with the lower functions of the cortex, whatever this expression may mean. Since the relative abundance of small pyramids is a test of the supremacy of the cortex, we may speak of them vaguely as concerned with its higher functions. But a surer test of the capacity of the cortex for the elaboration of the raw materials of thought which sensory nerves deliver to it is the relative abundance of the tissue which intervenes between its cells. The number of cell-bodies to be counted in a square millimetre of a section of a given thickness is smaller in Man than in a monkey, in a monkey than in a dog, and in a dog than in a rabbit. A comparison of the brains of various mammals in which particular sense-organs are either deficient or exceptionally well developed affords the clearest proof of the localization of sensory areas. This, if it were possible to make satisfactory measurements, would be by far the best class of evidence as to the part played by the several senses in an animal’s mental life. Unfortunately, measurement appears to be out of the question ; but a glance at a rabbit’s brain, placed by the side of a mole’s, shows that vision is localized in the occipital region. All marine mammals are destitute of the sense of smell; the brain of a dog, compared with that of a porpoise or a whale, shows that the sphenoidal region (cf. Fig. 25) is associated with this sense. The brain of an otter exhibits very clearly the area into which impulses arising in 350 THE BODY AT WORK the nerve-endings of the sensory bristles of the cheek are poured. | “Nihil est in intellectu quod non prius in sensu fuerit.” The organ of the intellect is the cortex of the great brain, a sheet of grey matter which has developed in connection with the various sense-organs. The cerebral hemisphere of an infant is merely an extension of the nerve-tissue associated with its sense-organs. Such it remains in a microcephalous idiot. In the lower animals its capacity of growth after birth is very small. But in a normal child the inflow of impressions through sense- organs, the experience acquired regarding itself and its sur- roundings, education, whether accidental or directed, causes the extension of nerve-tissue from the sensory areas into the expansible intervening zones. There is still some uncertainty as to the nature of the sensa- tions received in the excitable area. They may be termed “‘ kinesthetic ’’ (sensations connected with movement) without more exact definition. Some physiologists consider that tactile sensations, as well as the obscure sensations, originated in the nerve-endings in muscles, around tendons, or on joint-surfaces, are distributed to the areas, which, when stimulated, are shown to represent fingers, hand, arm, and other parts of the body. Others have sought, though with doubtful success, for a tactile area, independent of the kinesthetic centres. When first discovered, these centres were termed “ motor,” and still this term may be retained, on the understanding that it does not imply that the exchanges which occur in the kinesthetic centres are of a different nature to those which take place else- where. The region which they occupy has become the motor area of the cortex because voluntary movement is possible only under the guidance of sensations of movement. A sound or a retinal image may prompt the movement; but the part of the temporal region, or of the occipital region in which the sound- movement exchange or sight-movement exchange occurs must act through the motor area by opening kinesthetic-move- ment arcs. Destruction of a part of the kinesthetic cortex causes in Man and the higher apes permanent paralysis for the movements directed by the spot destroyed. In lower animals the definition of the movement centres is vague, and their removal produces only temporary results. Their mastery 351 over the muscles is less complete than in the higher apes and Man. | Practically nothing is known with regard to localization of function in the association-zones, with the exception of the localization of the centres for words ; but this exception is so remarkable as to suggest that if there were any other faculties, interference with which caused defects as distinct as those which characterize disorders of speech, it would be found that the association-zones are made up of definite centres. As the evidence stands with regard to the broadest continental divisions, we can merely state that it points, although not very clearly, to the connection of the frontal zone, the region in front of the kinesthetic area, with ideas of personality, of other zones with ideas of environment. Injury to the frontal region has in certain cases resulted in the victim’s losing his knowledge of himself, his name, and his relation to his family. On the other hand, gunshot wounds and other definite injuries have in a large number of cases destroyed portions of the cortex behind the forehead without causing any recognizable intellec- tual change. It is quite certain that this part of the brain performs no functions which are of a different, or, as it is often called, higher order than those of other association-zones. It has been stated that disease of the zone which intervenes beween the visual and auditory areas is more likely to cause hallucina- tions, disease of the frontal zone delusions. A patient fancies in the one case that he sees things that are not there, or hears voices when no one is speaking ; in the other case he imagines himself a king ; but evidence connecting localized disease with -mental derangement is very scanty. The functional disturb- ance which causes lunacy is usually of a general character ; or, if local to begin with, it becomes general before the death of the patient makes possible the examination of his brain. Derangements of speech throw a flood of light upon the organization and manner of working of the association-zones ; and, owing to the accident of the continuation of the line of the carotid artery by the middle cerebral artery, which supplies the speech centre, there is no other spot in the cortex so likely to be thrown out of gear. A little plasma coagulates on one of the cardiac valves, or about an atheromatous spot in the aorta. Detached by the blood-stream, it is shot into one of the branches = y 352 ‘THE BODY AT WORK of the middle cerebral artery, which it plugs, causing apoplexy. A larger or smaller number of muscles on the opposite side of the body are paralysed. If the plugging occurs on the left side of the brain, it is accompanied by aphasia; but only if it occurs on the left side, owing to the fact—perhaps the most Kinaesthetic centres WAKES : —4 Cerebellum Spinal cord Fic. 25.—THE SURFACE OF THE LEFT CEREBRAL HEMISPHERE, CEREBELLUM, AND MEDULLA OBLONGATA. Sensory areas are enclosed by broken lines ; certain centres in the association-zones are marked by dots. The sensory area of smell is on the inner aspect of the brain ; so also is the area of vision which borders the calcarine and retrocalcarine fissures, and only rarely extends on to the external surface, as shown in the diagram. ‘The sensory area of hearing is largely hidden within the fossa of Sylvius, the opening into which is indicated by the dark line above it. The kinesthetic-sensory areas for the various muscles of the body occupy the territory between the dotted line in front and the bottom of the fissure of Rolando behind. ‘They do not extend on to the posterior wall of this fissure. It is impossible at present to define the boundaries of any of the centres in the association-zones. remarkable in connection with the localization of speech— that only on the left side is the cortex trained to utter words. In course of time the patient may recover the power of speaking, but not until he has, with almost as much labour as in child- hood, educated the right side to do the work. There are four THE NERVOUS SYSTEM 353 speech-centres, quite distinct one from the other. Near the visual area is the centre for seeing words, or rather the centre for seeing the meaning of words. If this centre be diseased, a written word is merely a crooked line. Behind the auditory area is the centre for recognizing the meaning of words heard. If it is interfered with, the most endearing or commanding phrases produce no more impression on the hearer than a bird’s song. In front of the hand-area—its localization is less certain than that of the other three—is the centre for writing. In it are associated words heard or seen, with the movements necessary for the making of letters. In the centre first referred to, as being the one most often thrown out of gear, which lies in front of the area for the mouth and throat, words heard or seen are translated into movements of the parts which give them sound. No other actions illustrate so clearly the “law of neural habit.” In the infant’s brain sounds of words are distinguished from other sounds. They are associated with the objects which they name. Movements of the mouth and throat, made at first ineffectively, blunder- ingly, succeed after a time in securing the thing of which they sound the name to the child’s satisfaction. Thus, two centres are gradually established in his mind. Sounds and ideas of things are associated in the one ; words and ideas of the movements necessary to their pronunciation in the other. Kither of the four speech-centres may be placed out of action without the others suffering. A man may be able to write without being able to read what he has written. He may read aloud, although apparently deaf to speech. He may be unable to write or unable to speak, although understanding what he reads or hears. Aphasia, when partial, illustrates still further the law of neural habit. The ability to remember nouns, especially proper names, is most easily lost. Few are the people who, as age advances, do not suffer from this failing. Even the names which are most familiar elude the memory. From one point of view this is strange. Nouns-substantive are the words first learned. Of all words they have the most definite objective association. But it is just their definiteness which makes them difficult of approach when the apparatus of mind is working badly. There are so few paths by which they can be reached. Their mental associations are limited. A patient ; 23 THE BODY AT WORK a who is recovering cont the effects of a inion which Ps ren- 7 dered him partially aphasic may be able to recall adjectives — when he cannot recall nouns. He may say, “Give me the black,” when he wants ink, and “ Give me the white,” when — ; he needs paper. Or he may retain control of verbs. “ Where is the what I put on—what I think with ?” may be the circumlocution for hat. Psychologists explain the voluntary rode: of a move- ment as the setting flowing of a sensori-motor current. LEvery- one agrees that it is impossible to think of the impulses which produce movement as originating without sensory antecedents. Hence psychologists picture the nerve-current as originating on the sensory side. Kinesthetic images of the sensations which will result from the movement are described as being called up in the mind by the agitation of the part of the brain which, by association, is linked with the neurones which dis- charge impulses to the appropriate spots in the grey matter of the spinal cord. The idea of movement flows over to the muscles. But this conception of the relation of mind to body assumes too much. It postulates an existent mind in which the images of movement-sensations—the memories, that is to say, of the sensations which previously accompanied move- ment—are stored. The study of the apparatus of mind does not warrant this assumption of an existent mind. It finds nothing in the nervous system but apparatus. There is no mind existent in the brain during sleep. It would appear to be sufficient to describe the origination of a voluntary move- ment as the opening of the channels which convey the afferent impulses which are ceaselessly pouring into grey matter from nerve-endings in and about muscles into efferent channels. Our conception of the number of sensations which reach the realm of consciousness is ludicrously restricted by our inability _to pay attention to more than one sensation at a time—a_ restriction, it is needless to remark, which is imperative in the interests of consistency of behaviour. Two personalities paying attention to different sequences of sensations would give in- compatible orders. One would command the muscles to cause the body to recline ; the other would direct them to make it stand up. From myriads of sense-organs impulses are con- tinuously rippling through the cortex of the brain. The term ud eae PS ey ee oe sy v = RS Sta are ae ticy Deeb ok nae me runt Spy oy a SS ee re rs - ie~ ets Pe em ee ‘ Pan hata Se eT a a ee a Sie eae eel. ae Gee Eee) | oe . or. Rhea a Fs

| Probably even such figures as these would be thrown intel the shade if we could estimate the minimum amount of human effuvium which will enable a dog to follow his master’s trail. Explanations have been sought in alterations in the vibrations — of molecules of air caused by the presence amongst them of — relatively heavy molecules of volatile substances ; but the diffi- — culty of accounting for the generation of nerve-impulses in the — sensory cells remains as great as ever. The hairs borne by ~ olfactory cells are so short that it is impossible that they should project beyond the film of moisture on the surface of the mem- brane. This seems to preclude an answering vibration. Yet an increase in the thickness of this layer and in its density, due to the presence in it of mucus secreted during a catarrh, renders the sense-cells incapable of responding to odorous particles. Smell in an animal is not a test of the quality of the air it is breathing, but a source of information as to the direction in which it may seek its prey ; or, although far more rarely, as to the direction from which the advance of a foe is to be feared. Hunting animals depend for the most part on the nose. Hunted. animals rely chiefly on the eye. If we attempt to analyse our smell-sensations, we find that we can pick out a number of varieties which appear so unlike as to have nothing in common: Putrid meat, burning india- rubber, sulphuretted hydrogen, ammonia, roses, onions, lemon verbena, methylated spirit. Everyone can make for himself a list of typical odours which seem to have specific qualities— odours so distinct that he never confuses one with another. He can also class together scents about which he is often un- certain. The type-odours he can distinguish when present in a mixture ; whereas odours which are less distinct reinforce or modify one another. It has been found, by careful experiment, that certain type-odours even tend to neutralize each other. Musk and bitter almonds, for example, if present in small quantities and properly proportioned, produce a very dim sensation, whether supplied as a mixture to both nostrils, or the one assertive odour to one nostril and the other to the other. This last observation is of great importance. It proves that their mutual destruction does not occur on the olfactory mem- brane. It is not due to physical interference. The sensation ‘SMELL AND TASTE 367 of musk is delivered to one side of the brain, the sensation of ___ bitter almonds to the other ; but when attention is directed to these two sensations there is found a quality in the one which is irreconcilable with the quality of the other. In certain persons and under certain pathological conditions, sensitiveness to particular odours, or groups of odours, is absent, while for the rest the sense is normal. Methylated spirit, prussic acid and mignonette, constitute a group which not infrequently drops out. Instances have also been reported of persons unable to smell vanilla (to which some are hyper- sensitive), and of others insensitive to violets, although normally sensitive to the scents of other flowers. The notes sounded in consciousness extend over a long gamut ; but there are reasons for thinking that the number of keys on the clavier which odoriferous substances strike is limited. Eleven is the number provisionally adopted. The effect in consciousness varies according as one key or another is struck, or several at the same time with varying degrees of force. - Many attempts have been made to associate the sensation- qualities of the various odours with the chemical or physical properties of their odorants, with but little success as yet. To excite the sense of smell, a gas must be at least a little heavier than air. No volatile body, it is stated, is so heavy as to be odourless ; on the contrary, speaking generally, heavy molecules are more stimulating than light. The quality of a smell-sensa- tion would therefore appear to depend upon the period of vibra- tion of the molecules of the substance which evokes it; but, as already stated, a consideration of the apparatus which responds to stimulation by odoriferous particles does not help us to an understanding of the way in which the particles act upon it. Taste is far more limited in its range of sensations than smell. The back of the tongue is sensitive to bitters, the tip to sweets and salts, the sides to acids. Mixtures of these qualities are distinctly analysable by the sense of taste. Our sensations of taste donotfuse. Slight differences in the way in which the organs on the different parts of the tongue react to stimulation enable us to recognize that a sapid substance is a mixture. When, with a great flourish of trumpets, saccharin was intro- duced as a safe sweetener for gouty people, an attempt was made to provide them with saccharin-sweetened jam. The suppression, followed by nauseating sweetness. The se a a organs which subserve the sense of taste are clusters of fusiform. , epithelial cells, collected in “ taste-bulbs” (Fig. 26). Each a gustatory cell bears a minute bristle, which projects through — PSO Ori [| | | | hS CGR rrr Fic. 26.—HIGHLY MAGNIFIED SECTION THROUGH THE WALL OF A CIRCUMVALLATE PAPILLA OF THE TONGUE, SHOWING TWO TASTE-BULBS. i) < These sense-organs are groups of elongated epithelial cells, set vertically to the surface. Their cells are of two kinds—the one fusiform, slender, bearing each a bristle-like process which projects through a minute pore left between the superficial cells of the general epithelium ; the other thicker and wedge-shaped. Nerve-fibres are connected with the fusiform cells. the pore left by the cells of the surrounding epithelium which constitute a globular case for the bulb. As in the nose, eye, and ear, a second thicker variety of epithelial cell is also present. The nerve-fibres of the taste-bulbs are not, as in the olfactory membrane, processes of their cells, but branches of the fifth nerve which ramify amongst them. On the back of the tongue taste-bulbs are much more numerous than elsewhere. They are ~~ ‘7 hw q ~ SMELL AND TASTE | 369 not as sensitive as the cells of the olfactory membrane ; never- theless, they enable us to detect 1 part of quinine in 2,000,000 parts of water. | - Sensations of taste and smell endure for a long time after stimulation, because the odorous or sapid substance remains in contact with the sense-organs. This accounts for the con- fusion into which a man is thrown if he sip alternately port and sherry. After a short time he cannot tell the one from the other. The organs are quickly fatigued, using the term loosely. How intolerable patchouli would be to the ladies who use it were it otherwise! If for some time one sniffs the odour of mignonette, it ceases to be recognizable ; whereas, turning to a rose, the olfactory membrane is found to be as sensitive as usual. When the sense is fatigued for a particular smell, it is dull for others of the same group, thus affording an opportunity of classifying smell-sensations according to their qualities ; but the method is difficult to apply. Taste-organs are greatly affected by temperature. Quinine is not tasted just after drinking ice-cold water. Alcohol, ether, or chloro- form paralyses the organs much in the same way. Castor-oil slips down the throat unnoticed if the mouth, just before swallowing it, has been rinsed with brandy or with a strong solu- tion of tincture of chloroform. Englishmen make but little use of their sense of smell. It might teach them much regarding the various emanations from putrid matter which are produced by bacterial action ; but, dreading drains, they decline to cultivate proficiency in the exercise of this sense. The nose is valued for the warning it gives of “nasty smells,”’ but is not allowed to analyse them. Burnt milk, soap-boilings, rancid oils, are taboo, because they are associated with bungling in the kitchen. With moderated ardour, we allow our sense of smell to distinguish foods and beverages, but we are not a race of epicures. The perfumes of flowers are classed as “ nice smells.”’ The idea of greediness is not associated with their enjoyment ; besides, they remind us of gardens, sunshine, pretty forms and colours. When bottled, musk, orange-blossom, violets, lavender, are valued not so much for their own sweetness, as for their singular efficiency in obscuring nasty smells. Few persons practise the recognition and distinction of even pleasant odours. Very 24 370 THE BODY AT WORK few, on first coming across a scented herb or shrub, pay suffi- cient attention to its perfume to impress it on their memories. They note the shape of its leaves and the colour of its flowers, but they are unable to identify it by its odour when they meet with it again. It is not much to be wondered at, therefore, that this slighted sense tends to leave us after middle life. It has been asserted—and probably the statement is justified— that rarely is the olfactory bulb of a man over forty free from signs of atrophy. We have no statistics concerning the brains of Japanese, who regard the sense of smell as one of the chief avenues of pleasure ; but it may be that in this respect their brains present a contrast to our own. Yet the deadening of the sense is scarcely noticed, since its results are of little con- sequence as compared with those which follow loss of sight or loss of hearing. Many a man, as he grows older, declares that the cook of his club has lost his cunning, or frankly asserts that he “‘ no longer cares for kickshaws. Cold beef, beer, and pickles, are good enough for him.” He little suspects that his palate has lost its power of distinguishing the flavours of dainty meats and wines. Others continue to be exacting, because their imaginations still endow food with the qualities which they remember, just as people eat preserved asparagus or tinned peas because they look—however little they taste—like the gifts of Spring. Taste accompanies the reception of food in the mouth. We have no knowledge of the situation of our own olfactory mem- branes, and therefore we suppose that a flavour, whether it be due to stimulation of taste-bulbs or olfactory membrane, is in the mouth. The odour of a flower we mentally project to a distance, because we associate the sight of a flower with its perfume. A dog, able to judge the freshness or stale- ness of a scent, must project its sensations of smell in the same way in which we project our sensations of sight. It forms an estimate, of a sort, of the time that it will take in reaching the source of the scent. Its excitement increases as the trail grows fresher. Taste and smell are heavily laden with affective tone. When disagreeable, the feeling which they evoke is near akin to pain. It may gather head until, like hunger, it causes the discharge of motor neurones; but under its influence ‘SMELL AND TASTE 371 food is ejected, instead of preparation being made for its reception. Taste and smell are senses which afford us no information with regard to time or space. They give rise to massive sensa- tions. Such sensations, devoid of detail, produce a frame of mind rather than thought. The smell of tobacco does not distract attention. On the contrary, the steady flow of im- pulses to which it gives rise helps to inhibit, to subdue, the yapping of more exigent sensations. And since sensations of smell have no features of their own, they form a background to sensations of other kinds, entering with them into memory. No two scenes are exactly alike. One cannot recall another. But the scent of syringa is always the same. Wherever smelled, it opens the pathways in the brain in which were first associated a June evening and syringa, with a scene and a situation upon which memory loves to dwell. 24—2 CHAPTER XIII VISION THE eye is enclosed in a globe of fibrous tissue, of which the front part, or cornea, being transparent, admits light. The epithelial layer which covers the cornea, conjunctiva, is also transparent. No bloodvessels enter these colourless tissues, unless as the result of inflammation due to infection or to exposure to sunshine or dust. For nutrition they are depen- dent upon the plasma which, exuding from, and returning to, the vessels which surround them, circulates in their tissue- spaces. In advancing years, when the circulation is less brisk, a ring of opaque tissue, arcus senilis, encroaches on the cornea. In the interior of the globe, just behind the cornea, is a pro- jecting shelf, formed of a ring of tissue supported by buttresses, ciliary processes. It is continued inwards as the iris, a mus- cular curtain. The “ hyaloid membrane ” lines the back por- tion of the globe. Continued on the inner side of the ciliary processes, it splits into several layers, which pass, one in front of the lens, others to its edge, to which they are attached, and still another, very thin, behind it. Since it holds the lens in place, the anterior portion of the hyaloid membrane is known as its ‘‘ suspensory ligament.’ Thus the eyeball is divided into three chambers. The anterior is filled with watery lymph, aqueous humour. In it, resting on the anterior surface of the suspensory ligament of the lens, is the iris. The middle chamber contains the lens. The posterior chamber is filled with a liquid jelly, vitreous humour. By the contraction of the circular fibres of the iris, the aperture of the pupil is diminished, limiting the light which enters the globe. This adjustment occurs when the illumina- tion is bright. It is also brought into action for the purpose 372 VISION _ | 373 of cutting out divergent rays, which would not be clearly focussed when objects near at hand are looked at. The posterior surface of the iris and the inner surfaces of the ciliary processes are covered with dense black pigment. It is this pigment, showing through the uncoloured connective tissue and plain muscle-fibres of which the iris is composed, that gives their colour to grey and blue eyes. In many eyes the iris contains a brown pigment in its substance. Central Artery--°*" | of Retina Fic. 27.—HORIZONTAL SECTION THROUGH THE RIGHT EYE. The slight depression in the retina in the axis of the globe is the fovea centralis, or yellow spot the optic nerve pierces the ball to its inner or nasal side. The lens, with its suspensory ligament, separates the aqueous from the vitreous humour. On the front of the lens rests the iris, covered on its posterior surface wtih black pigment. On either side of the lens is seen a ciliary process, with the circular fibres of the ciliary muscle cut transversely, and its radiating fibres disposed as a fan. The back portion of the globe of the eye is covered with a curtain, the retina, formed by the spreading out of the fibres of the optic nerve in front of various layers of nerve-cells and the sensory cells of the organ of vision, rods and cones. The retina lies between the hyaloid membrane, which encloses the vitreous humour, and a layer of pigment which “ backs ”’ it, as a photographer backs a plate when he proposes to use it towards a source of light—to take a photograph of a window from within a room. The serrated margin of the retina is somewhat anterior to the equator of the eyeball. The pig- ment which backs the retina is contained in a sheet of cells , i oe hi, a Oe ee Se tee a ed 4 a. of Se Soe ae ee ey ‘ = ov so) ar a *. Aas x = lll 374 | THE BODY AT WORK | which belongs to the pouch of brain that extended outwards 4 towards the eye-pit (p. 334). Properly speaking, therefore, it — _ is a layer of the retina. Three sets of tissues take part in the development of the eyeball. (1) The epithelium covering the surface of the head is depressed as a pit, which gradually closes into a hollow sphere. This sphere, when its cavity is filled up, owing to the great elongation of the cells of its posterior half, becomes the lens. It breaks away from the rest of the epithelium of the surface, which clears to transparency as that part of the con- junctiva termed the “ corneal epithelium.” (2) The retina, as already stated, is a hollow outgrowth from the interbrain. i) SS Pee. @& (oA Qp ey QAADAS S A B Fig. 28.—DIAGRAMS SHOWING THE MODE OF FORMATION OF THE CRYSTALLINE LENS. A, A pit in the epithelium on the surface of the head has closed into a hollow sphere. 8B, The cells of the posterior wall of this sphere are growing forward, as the fibres of the lens pag traverse its whole thickness, with the exception of the cubical epithelium on its As this pouch approaches the lens, its anterior half is pushed back into the posterior half, forming a cup with a double wall. The anterior, or inner, sheet of the bowl of the cup develops into the nervous layers of the retina, the posterior sheet into its pigmented epithelium. (3) Connective tissues are trans- formed into the other constituents of the globe—cornea, iris, vitreous humour, etc. The globe is complete, except at a spot on the nasal side of its posterior pole where the optic nerve pierces it. The bloodvessels of the retina, entering with the optic nerve, ramify on its anterior surface. Under ordinary circumstances we ignore the shadows which they cast, as we ignore the blind- spot which coincides with the disc of insensitive tissue presented ous te ae of the pea’ nerve, and many other imperfec- tions ;. but it was shown by Purkinje many years ago that by a very simple manceuvre they may be forced upon our notice. By making use of Purkinje’s figures, it can be proved that the level in the retina at which undulations of light give rise to the impulses which evoke visual sensations coincides with the back of its anterior sheet—.e., with the layer of rods and cones. A person stares fixedly at a white sheet in a dimly lighted room while an assistant, by the help of a lens, focuses Fig. 29.—PURKINJE’S SHADOWS. A beam of light traversing the eyeball in the direction A throws a shadow of the vessel v, lying on the front of the retina, upon the sensitive layer at its back. When the light is moved from A to B the shadow moves from atob. The mind, supposing the shadow to be a dark mark on the nearest wall or screen, infers that this mark moves from A’ to B’. a strong light on the front of his eyeball, to the outer side of the cornea. The rays, traversing the white of the eye, throw shadows of the retinal vessels on the layers behind them ; but this not being the way in which light normally enters the eye- bali, the person experimented upon supposes that he sees the shadows in front of him. He mentally projects them on to the white sheet. The pattern of his retinal vessels appears on the sheet in grey streaks. When the spot of light is moved, the shadow-pattern shifts, and in the same direction; since, 376 THE BODY AT WORK as the retinal image is reversed, a movement from right to left is interpreted by consciousness as a movement from left to right. Given the angle through which the light is moved, and the apparent displacement of the shadows, it is a simple matter to calculate the distance behind the bloodvessels of the sensitive layer of the eye. So definite are Purkinje’s figures that the shadows of individual blood-corpuscles can be fol- lowed, and the rate at which they are moving in the capillaries of the retina calculated. The retina is the organ of vision. Cornea, iris, lens, vitreous . humour, are parts of the camera in which this sensitive screen is exposed ; and of the retina, the sensitive layer is the layer of rods and cones. Interest therefore centres in these struc- tures. They are disposed with the utmost regularity on the posterior surface of a thin, reticulated membrane—the outer limiting membrane. But rods and cones are only the outer halves of sensory cells, the inner portions of which, reduced to a minimum in thickness, except where they contain their nuclei, lie in the outer nuclear layer. Rods are the larger elements. Each consists of an outer segment, or limb, of relatively firm substance transversely striated, and liable to break into discs ; and an inner limb of much softer substance, again divisible into two parts, the outer longitudinally striated, the inner granular. Cones are almost identical in structure with rods, save that their outer limbs are much smaller, their inner limbs rather fuller. In frogs and various other animals, but not in Man, each cone contains at the junction of its two limbs a highly refracting globule of oil, often brightly coloured, red, yellow, or green. The layers in front of the rods and cones contain nervous elements accessory to them. In the “inner nuclear layer ” are the ganglion-cells of the retina, homologous with the cells of the ganglia on the posterior roots of spinal nerves; but, in the retina, bipolar and extremely minute. On either side of the rather thick layer occupied by the nuclei of these ganglion- cells (and of cells of other types which, for the sake of clearness, we omit) is a felt-work of nerve-filaments in which their two extremities arborize. The most internal, or anterior, layer consists of a single sheet of rather large collecting cells and of their axons, which stream towards the optic nerve. Each — 377 Utne Zep NP NEN \ (fenew Fig. 30.—THE RETINA IN VERTICAL SEOCTION—A, AFTER EXPOSURE TO BRIGHT LIGHT; B, AFTER RESTING IN THE DARK. The arrow shows the direction in which light traverses the retina. C, Retinal epithelium, with its pigmented fringe. 1, Layer of rods and cones, separated by the external limiting membrane from 2, the layer of the nuclei of the rods and cones. 3, The ganglion cells of the retina, which are homologous with the cells of the afferent root of a spinal nerve. Their peripheral axons ramify beneath the sensory epithelium (rods and cones and their nucleus-bearing segments), their central axons in 4, the inner molecular layer. D, Collect- ing cells on the front of the retina; aaa, their axons which conduct impulses to the brain ; b, an efferent fibre from the brain. 378 THE BODY AT WORK cone has its proper ganglion-cell, collecting cell, and efferent fibre. Rods are served in groups by ganglion-cells and col- lecting cells. From this it may be inferred that a cone is a sensory unit, an inference confirmed, as we shall show presently, by direct evidence. The connections of the rods show that they also are sensory elements, although it may be doubted whether they are sensory units. The optic nerve contains a very large number of fibres—about a million—all small, but some distinctly larger than the rest. The largest very probably belong to the collecting cells of rods. But the retina certainly does not contain a million collecting cells. A considerable residue of fibres is therefore unaccounted for. It is supposed that . they are afferent to the retina, but we have no knowledge regarding the nature of the impulses which descend from the brain. The retinal pigment is not merely a backing for the sensitive screen. It undoubtedly plays an important part in vision. That it is not essential is evident from the fact that albinos, whose eyes appear pink owing to the absence of pigment, and the consequent showing through of the blood in the exceedingly vascular membrane which lies behind the retina, can see; although their visual sense cannot be described as normal. They are exceptionally sensitive to an excess of light. We shall return to this subject after describing the differences in manner of functioning which distinguish rods from cones, differences so marked as to justify us in speaking of two kinds of vision. During twilight warm tones gradually fade out of the landscape ; cold blues and greys predominate. A time arrives when scarlet poppies look black, although yellow and blue flowers and green leaves can still be dimly distinguished. In full daylight colours are seen at their brightest in the high lights ; where the light is dim they tend to appear in different shades of grey. At night, if the sky is star-lit, all colours give place to a slightly bluish grey in the high lights, black in the shade. But a not very uncommon abnormality is night- blindness—inability to see at all when the light is not bright enough for the recognition of colours. In persons so affected the rods do not function ; for it is with the rods that we see in weak light. They record differences in intensity between the . VISLON “379 lower limit of their sensitiveness and the higher degree of brightness, at which they are superseded by cones ; but they afford no information regarding colour. Their monochrome is interpreted by the mind as a bluish grey, apparently because, since they are insensitive to red rays, the sensations of which they are the source are associated with the blue end of the spectrum. When the cones are stimulated very slightly, the reinforcing grey of the rods enables us to distinguish all other colours, save red, which appears black. In bright light the rods are in a permanent state of exhaustion ; they do not contribute to vision. Rods respond to stimulation more slowly than cones. This fact enables us, by a very pretty experiment, to distinguish the two kinds of vision. A disc of green paper about the size of a threepenny-bit is pasted on a red surface. Held at arm’s length in a room lighted by a single candle, the disc looks dull green when the gaze is directed at it; but if the gaze be directed 2 or 3 inches to one side of it, it appears brighter than before, but less distinct and almost grey. The explanation of this is to be found in the fact that at the posterior pole of the eye there is a shallow cup—fovea centralis—which carries cones only, without rods. This small depression is the area of direct vision, the only spot at which we see things quite distinctly. At the fovea the nuclei and nerve-cells of the retina are withdrawn from in front of the cones to the margin of the cup, in order that they may not interfere with the passage of light. The pit and the ring round it contain some yellow pigment. Hence it is usually termed the “ yellow spot.” When we are looking straight at the green disc, it is focussed on the yellow spot. It then excites a sensation of greenness; but since this is not reinforced by any rod-sensations, the green is dull. When it is focussed outside the yellow spot, it stimulates rods and the sparse cones which lie amongst them ; and the rods being more sensitive than cones to light of low intensity, the disc looks brighter. If, while the observer is still gazing fixedly at a spot to the side of the disc, the red paper be waved rapidly, but gently, to right and left, a brightish grey cover seems at each movement to slip off the dark green disc, and to regain its position a moment later, with a jump. The grey rod-sensation, developing more slowly than the green cone-sensation, is, as 380 THE BODY AT WORK it were, left behind. The two are separated at the moment when the paper starts to right or to left. Astronomers have long recognized that one of the smaller stars which catches the attention when they are not looking directly at it may be invisible when the gaze is directed to the spot where it ought to be. It was visible when focussed on rods, but it is not visible when focussed on cones. In most birds the retina shows cones alone. To anyone who for the first time enters a dovecote at night the experience is very curious. A candle is for him a sufficiently strong illuminant, but it does not give light enough to enable the pigeons to see. Although evidently alarmed by the noise made by the intruder, they allow themselves to be taken down from their perches — without making any attempt to escape. If, startled by the touch of a hand, they take to flight, they fly against the wall. Pigeons are night-blind. The retina of an owl bears chiefly rods, the outer limbs of which are exceptionally long. The outer limbs of the rods are coloured reddish purple. This colour is quickly bleached by light. If a frog which has been kept for a short time in the dark be decapitated, its head fixed for ten minutes in a situation in which a window is in front of it, then carried to a photographic dark-room, where an eye is taken out by red light, opened, and the retina removed, a print of the window will be seen upon it. Such an optogram may be fixed by dipping the retina in alum. The retina is easily detached from its pigment-layer. If it has been bleached by exposure to light, it regains its “ visual purple”’ when again placed in contact with its pigment. Evidently the visual purple is renewed from the pigment which lies behind (and around) the rods. From the cells of the pigment-layer a fringe of streaming processes depends amongst the outer limbs of the rods and cones (Fig. 30). Ina dull light the processes hang but a short way down ; in a bright iight they react almost to the outer limit- ing membrane. They supply pigment to the rods, but their relation to cones is not understood. It is clear, however, that the cones, although they are not coloured, are dependent upon the pigment-fringe, since they always remain in contact with it. Their inner limbs elongate in the dark, lifting them to the VISION 381 pigment, and shorten in bright light. These movements may | merely indicate that the cones require a backing of pigment, but it would seem more probable that, like the rods, they absorb a substance which is sensitive to light, although we cannot recognize it by its colour. The responsiveness of the rods to light is due to visual purple. As every lady is aware, colours, especially mauves and lilacs, are bleached by light. The chemical change affected by light in the colour of the outer limbs of the rods is the stimulant which originates impulses in the nerve-fibres con- nected with them, and it is generally believed that cones—the more highly specialized sensory cells—are stimulated in the same way. Visual purple is particularly abundant in all animals that range at night, with the exception of the bat. But its absence in the bat does not militate against the theory that it is the cause of night-vision, for it has been shown that a blind bat flies with almost as much freedom, and avoids obstacles—even threads stretched across the room—with as much skill as one that can see. It is guided by the bristles of its cheek. So, too, is the cat, which has the reputation of being able to see in the dark. Undoubtedly a cat’s eye is an exceptionally efficient organ in dim light, just as it is excep- tionally sensitive to sunshine—it is provided with an iris which contracts the pupil almost to a pinhole—but the cat trusts to the bristles of its cheek for information regarding the things which block its path. Most of the peculiarities which distinguish the reactions of the eye from those of other sense-organs can be explained by its mode of stimulation—the initiation of a nerve-current by a chemical change. No stimulus, if sufficiently strong, can be too brief. The retina reacts to an electric spark in the same way as a photographic plate ; but, unlike the plate, the retina is restored to its previous condition of sensitiveness in about one-tenth of asecond. A visual sensation lasts about one-tenth of a second. This prolongation of the sensation is, however, a mental, not a retinal, effect. The mind continues to see an object which has been illuminated by a flash until the retina is again in a condition to send brainwards a second impulse. Were our sensations coincident in duration with the stimulation of our sense-organs, we should live in a flickering cinemato- 389 THE BODY AT WORK _ graph. When one is watching a moving point of light—the glowing end of a match, for example—the prolongation of sensation has its disadvantages; the moving point is inter- preted as a streak of light. If the illumination be very brilliant, the object seen may give rise to a prolonged after- image. A glance at the sun leaves in the mind for seconds, or even for minutes, the image of a glowing disc. Sensations due to stimulation of the yellow spot last longer than those which originate in the peripheral retina. If, in a train, one is being carried at a certain pace, past a fence composed of up- right palings, one sees the separate slats until the eyes are directed towards them, when they fuse into a continuous screen. The phenomena of negative or complementary images are of retinal origin. The bright image of the sun, if the stimulus has not been too violent, gives place to a black disc. If one closes the eyes after staring at a window, a black surface crossed by bright lines is seen in place of a white surface with dark frames to the panes. If, after staring at a red surface, one looks at the ceiling, a green patch is seen ; after yellow, blue. Every colour has its complement, which may be determined in this way. There is much uncertainty as to the exact terms in which this phenomenon is to be accounted for, but little doubt as to its being due to the peculiar mode of reaction of the retina to light. Chemical substances which have been used up have to be restored, and during the period in which they are coming back to what may be termed a neutral condition the retina delivers to the brain impulses of the opposite sign. Contrasts which are experienced simultaneously are more difficult to understand than those which appear successively. In Fig. 31 the half of the grey cross which is surrounded by black appears brighter than the half which lies on white paper. A grey cross on a red background looks green; on a green background, red ; on yellow, blue ; on blue, yellow. If green is on red, it looks greener than if it is on white or black. These simultaneous contrasts are seen best when the strength of the colours is reduced by covering them with tissue-paper. It is as if activity of any one part of the retina is accompanied by activity of the opposite sign in the remainder. But it is unsafe, in explaining our various sensations, to lay too much stress on the mode of stimulation. The mind judges sensations in VISION 383 the light of previous experience. In anatomical language, the effect of sensations upon the personality depends upon the paths which impulses follow in the brain, and the associations which have been established by previous impulses which have followed the same paths. The retina enables us to distinguish tone and colour. By the variations in tone, the juxtapositions of light and shade, we recognize form. All streams of impulses which do not present tone-variations—do not, that is to say, reproduce the details of a scene—are interpreted in terms of colour. Every child discovers that the tedium of the intervals during which it is proper that his eyes should be closed may be relieved by pressing his knuckles against the lids. Although Fic. 31.—SIMULTANEOUS CONTRAST. The shading of the two V’s is exactly similar ; but the figure in half-tone on black appears brighter than the figure in half-tone on a white ground. the world is shut out, a phosphene offers itself for his considera- tion—a yellow or white disc of irregular form with a red margin, changing into lilac bordered with green, and then into yellowish-green with a blue edge. Such, if my recollection can be trusted, were the pictures which I used to see as a boy ; but no adjustment of pressure calls them forth with anything like the same vividness now. All the senses show a tendency to rebound after activity, exhibiting contrast-phenomena ; but the contrasts of vision are more marked and varied than those of the other senses, as everyone who is curious in the observation of his own sensations is aware. Negative after-images are generally referred to the retina ; but various other kinds of after-image and contrast- phenomena must be attributed to the judgments passed by the \ 384 mind upon the sensations which it receives ; and not to physical changes in sense-organs. Positive after-images are well-marked appearances, although less common, perhaps, than the phe- nomena of reversal of sensation of which we have just written. On waking in the morning, one looks at the window ; shifting the gaze to the ceiling, an after-image of the window appears, just as one saw it, with bright panes and dark frame. The ‘dark adapted eye,” being exceptionally sensitive, yields the same persistent positive after-image as the eye in its usual con- dition yields, after being directed towards the sun at mid-day. Movement-after-images can be explained only by referring them to misdirection of judgment. If the gaze is fixed on a rock close beside a waterfall, then shifted to a bank covered with grass or bushes, the part of the bank which occupies the lateral part of the field of vision appears to rush upwards, reversing the movement of the water. When the gaze has been fixed upon falling water—a narrow stream sparkling in sunlight—a central strip of the field moves upwards, the margins remaining stationary. If one stares at the spot on the surface of a basin of water on which drops are falling from a tap, and then looks at the floor, it is seen to contract towards the spot looked at, reversing the movement of the ripples in the basin. These observations reveal a fact of great import- ance in the physiology of vision. It is, probably, impossible truly to fix the gaze. The muscles of the eyeball keep the retinal field in constant movement—larger movements with minute oscillations superposed. When, as in watching a waterfall, movement has for a time taken a definite direction, its cessation is judged to mean reversal. The anatomical unit of sensation is a cone. The fovea centralis, the only part of the retina capable of receiving sensa- tions sufficiently discrete for reading, contains cones alone. If the gaze be directed but a very few millimetres on to the white margin of the page, letters lose their form. In the fovea the centre of one cone is 3-6 » distant from the centre of the next. Two stars are visible as separate stars if they subtend an angle of at least 60 seconds with the eye. Their images on the retina are then 4 » apart. Parallel white lines ruled on black paper, held at such a distance as causes them to subtend angles of 60 seconds with the eye, appear not straight but ‘VISION © 385 wavy, showing that their images are taken up, not by a con- tinuous substance, but by the mosaic of cones. So far the explanation of the visual unit is strictly anatomical; but it must be added that trained observers can recognize the separateness of objects which subtend angles of much less than 60 seconds—not more than 5 or 6 seconds. This can be accounted for only on the hypothesis that images far closer together than the width of a cone produce a specific effect in passing across the anatomical unit. In 1807 Thomas Young, the physicist, formulated a theory to account for colour-vision. He supposed that the retina contains three kinds of apparatus—a, 6, and c—each especially responsive to a particular kind of light, all three slightly stimulated by rays of all colours. (Young imagined three kinds of nerve, but modern supporters of his theory suppose three different substances chemically changed by light.) A prism spreads out the rays which are combined in white light into a band in the order of their wave-lengths—those which have the longest wave-length (0-8 ») and the slowest rate of vibration (381 billions to the second) at one end, those which have the shortest wave-length (0-4 ») and the most rapid vibration (764 billions to the second) at the other: between these two extremes every intermediate grade of length and rapidity. These are a mere fraction—a small group—of the waves which the ether transmits, but they are all that we can see. The long, slow vibrations give rise to sensations which we describe as red; the short, rapid vibrations we describe as violet. Our names for the tints which intervene are singularly old-fashioned and unsatisfactory, but all persons agree that they recognize in the spectrum a certain number of definite colours. Some normal-sighted persons say twelve, others eighteen. It is largely a question of terminology. Many considerations show that it is quite unnecessary to imagine that the retina is affected in a different kind of way by every kind of light, or by each ,of several groups of waves. If the red of the spectrum is mixed with yellow, we receive an impression of orange, which is identical with the impression produced by waves of the mean length of red and yellow ; orange and green give yellow; yellow and blue, green. Any two complementary colours yield white. By taking three 25 duly mixed, not white light a but light 0 of any obbee ti, although not of spectral purity, since it is mixed with whites Young considered that all the conditions of colour-vision would — be satisfied, all our various sensations provided for, if the retina contain three kinds of apparatus which light, according ~ to its quality, affects in varying degrees ; and with this theory of three kinds of apparatus—a, 6, and c—the theory of three elementary or fundamental colour-sensations is indissolubly linked. The colour x produces its intensest effect when a is stimulated, with the least possible stimulation of b and c; y is the reaction of 6, z of c. Recent studies of the curves of intensity give us the tints of 2, y, and z as carmine-red, apple- green, and ultramarine blue. The blending of sensations is illustrated with the well-known colour-top. But perhaps the most striking proof that three elementary colour-sensations are adequate to produce our visual world is afforded by photographs taken with the three- colour method. Three plates are exposed—(a) behind a red screen, (6) behind a greenish-yellow screen, (c) behind a blue screen. ‘They are fixed in such a way that the portions acted upon by light are rendered insoluble, whereas the rest of the film can be dissolved away ; a is then stained red, 0 greenish yellow, c blue. The three are superposed, and the result appears to the eye as an exact reproduction of the subject of the photograph in all its hues. It shows every shade of orange and green and violet. It is as bright—that is to say, as full of white light—as the original. Various objections may, however, be brought against Young’s theory. Of these, the most weighty are: (1) The retina does not contain three kinds of apparatus, as Young sup- posed; nor can we find three kinds of photochemical substances, as required by the theory in its modern form. If we could find them, a fresh difficulty would arise; for we have no reasons for supposing that one and the same nerve-ending can receive stimuli of three different kinds. (2) The theory offers ~no explanation of negative after-images—the complementary colours experienced when the eye is closed after staring at a brightly coloured object. (3) It does not adequately account for the various deficiencies of colour-blindness. VISION 387 It is well recognized that there are various degrees of colour- blindness, and that the colour-vision of persons considered normal presents different grades of refinement. Nevertheless, - the abnormalities of colour-blind persons are so marked that cases fall into definite classes. Those whose cones do not function—which means that their yellow spots are either undeveloped or diseased—see all things grey. They are totally colour-blind. Excluding these, the colour-blind may be grouped in one or other of two divisions—(a) those who confuse red and green, (b) those who confuse yellow and blue. One person out of every thirty-five is red-green blind. The pro- portion is even higher if males only are considered, showing how very unfortunate is our choice of warning signals. A man who is red-green blind cannot tell the port from the starboard. light. Blue-yellow blindness is, on the other hand, extremely rare. According to Young’s theory, colour-blindness is due to the absence of one of the three sets of visual apparatus. But cases do not altogether conform to this hypothesis. We knew an amateur water-colourist, since deceased, who derived intense pleasure from the beauties of Nature, and showed no mean skill in reproducing them with his brush, notwithstanding the fact that he was red-green blind. Each night his sister arranged his paint-box for him, and only rarely did he use vermilion to fill in a foreground of lush green grass. But this mistake, when he made it, did not destroy his own satisfaction in the picture. It was clear that red had a value for him, although he confused it with green. It is impossible for a normal person to see through the eye of one who is colour-blind, and there is no other means of comparing his sensations with our own. The mistakes which the colour-blind make in sorting coloured objects and in naming mixtures of light selected from various parts of the spectrum show the range of their de- ficiency, but give us no information regarding the qualities of the sensations which they retain. The test of colour-sensitiveness usually employed is the grading of a large number of wools of different tint. The order in which the colours should be arranged is not a matter of opinion. They must be placed in the order in which they occur in the spectrum—.e., arranged according to their wave- lengths. In the cases of colour-blindness which are most fre- 25—2 388 quently met with the defect may be described as due to an absence of the sense of redness, or as an absence of the sense of greenness. The two conditions can be distinguished. But since the eye is not dark for red (although in certain cases vision is very weak for the red end of the spectrum) or dark for green, the abnormality cannot be adequately accounted for on structural grounds. It is not explicable on the hypo- thesis that one of three sets of responsive sense-organs (or nerve-fibres) or photochemical substances is absent from the eye. Again, it is generally agreed that the sensations of white, yellow, and blue of the red-green colour-blind are similar to those of normal persons. This is not in harmony with the ~ theory of the omission from their eyes of one of three pieces of colour-apparatus. Professor Hering, of Leipsic, adopting the generally accepted view that light effects chemical changes in substances contained in the retina, to which changes stimulation of nerve-endings is due, formulated a theory of colour-vision which many physio- logists prefer to Young’s. He imagines that the retina con- tains three kinds of pigment, each of which is, as he believes all living substance to be, in a constant state of change. It is at the same time being built up and destroyed. Using the terms which connote the opposite directions of metabolism, the pigment is simultaneously undergoing anabolism and katabolism ; the two processes, when the retina is at rest, maintaining equilibrium. When light acts upon either of the substances, it hastens, according to its quality, either the one process or the other ; and the chemical change, whether it be constructive or destructive, stimulates the endings of optic nerves. Hering assumes, therefore, that there are six elemen- tary qualities of visual sensation—red, green, yellow, blue, white, black. Red, yellow, white are due to anabolism of the visual substances ; green, blue, black are due to their kata- bolism. The installation of yellow amongst the unanalysable colours is a relief to many minds. It is almost impossible to think of yellow as a compounded colour. White also, we feel, is not a compounded colour, despite our knowledge that a prism scatters from it all the hues of the rainbow. Black, many per- sons assert, gives them a definite sensation, and not merely a sense of rest. (Parenthetically, it may be observed that the 389 feeling that a colour is pure or mixed is not to be trusted. It _ may be based upon the chromatic aberration of the eye, or it may be reminiscent of the paint-box. We know that we cannot make yellow by mixing red and green pigments, hence we feel that it is pure. Of green we are not by any means sure ; gamboge and Prussian blue come into our minds.) Except when the light which falls upon the retina is giving rise to one of the four pure colour-sensations, all three substances are simul- taneously affected, although one may be undergoing katabolism while the other two are being built up, or vice versa. Hering accounts for simultaneous contrast by assuming that the activity of any one part of the retina induces an opposite kind of change in the remainder, and especially in the vicinity of the primarily active part. When a certain patch is developing a sensation of red, the rest of the retina develops a sensation of green. The great merit of the theory is, however, to be found in its offering an explanation of complementary after-images. The green patch seen with closed eyes after one has stared at a red object is due to the rebound of metabolism. In returning to a condition of chemical equilibrium the retinal substance acts as a stimulant which evokes the antagonistic colour. But it is a theory which makes very large assumptions. It assumes, for example, the possibility of the existence of a substance which is built up by light from one end of the spectrum, and decomposed by light from its centre. Not that Hering regards the existence of three retinal substances as essential to his theory. He is prepared to transfer to the brain the seat of the substances, or the substance, which, by their, or its, anabolism and katabolism, produces antagonistic colour-perceptions ; but in this he is abandoning physiology for metaphysics. We have no warrant for imagining that there exists in the brain any substance which, by undergoing physical changes of various kinds, produces various psychical effects. The problem to be solved is physiological. Rays of light of different wave- lengths excite the retina to discharge impulses which are variously distributed in the brain. The effects which they produce in consciousness depend upon their distribution. The impulses to which the longest rays give rise evoke sensa- tions of red, those due to the shortest, sensations of violet. 390 “THE BODY AT WORK — And what is true of the retina as a whole is true, apparently, of — each individual cone. In what way does light act upon a cone? It is one of the most fascinating problems in physi- ology. Round it our thoughts revolve whenever we are trying to form conceptions of the nature of stimulation, sensation, and perception. Each of the two theories which we have ex- pounded above helps to group together certain of the more striking phenomena of colour-vision, but neither gives a satis- fying explanation of their causation. The sensitiveness of the retina is in a remarkable degree adjusted to the intensity of the light. When a dark room is entered, the pupil dilates ; but one’s power of distinguishing objects continues to increase after the pupil has reached its maximum size. At the end of ten minutes the eye may be twenty-five times as sensitive as it was when the room was entered. This adaptation to darkness is due in large degree to the substitution of rods for cones as the organs on which vision chiefly depends. But it cannot be wholly due to this, since it occurs when one is working with a red light. Probably the red used in a “ dark room ”’ is not sufficiently near the end of the spectrum to be completely without influence upon visual purple, but it is a colour to which rods are comparatively insensitive. Other evidence also points to an adaptation of cones as well as of rods. Accommodation of the eye for distance is brought about by a mechanism which allows the lens to change in shape. It becomes more convex when a near object is looked at than it was when adjusted for an unlimited distance, which is its con- dition when the eye is at rest. Adjustment for near objects involves muscular action, and is accompanied by a sense of effort, however slight. Whilst the eye is at rest the lens is mechanically compressed against the anterior layer of its sus- pensory ligament. Accommodation for near vision is effected by the ciliary muscle, which is placed in the shelf of tissue which projects into the interior of the eyeball. This muscle is made up of a ring of circular fibrés, and to the outer side of this, of fibres which radiate backwards and outwards. The longi- tudinal, or radiating, fibres obtain their purchase by attach- ment to the firm wall of the globe just beyond the cornea. They spread into the front of the loose chorioid membrane io, ss 1 iP “ et ‘ - > A ¥ Vv B Vv y as y eee re L \ (eat i Ge eas NY ro ‘4 eS Fig. 33. A The normal eyeball, in which, when the ciliary muscle is relaxed, parallel rays are brought to a focus on the retina. B, A hypermetropic eyeball. Its depth being less than normal, parallel rays are not brought to a focus on the retina when the eye is adjusted for distant vision without the aid of a convex glass. C, A myopic eyeball. Its depth being more than normal, a concave lens is needed to diminish the convergence of parallel rays. or may accompany insufficient length or too great length of the optic axis. It is due to unequal curvature of the cornea. Usually the curvature is sharper in the vertical than in the horizontal meridian (cf. p. 269) ; as a consequence, points in a vertical line are focussed in front of points in a horizontal line. : é fal) eat Pei, be en 2° ee — — / VISION | | 393 Cylindrical glasses, not lenses, are required to correct this defect. And here it may be well to call attention to the fact that rays of light are more sharply refracted by the surface of _ the cornea than they are by the crystalline lens. The lens has a high index of refraction (1-45), but it does not lie in air (the index of refraction of which is 1), but between two humours which have about the same index as water—namely, 1-336. The bending by the combined action of the cornea and the lens of rays of light which come from a source so distant that they may be considered as parallel brings them to a focus on the retina, when the lens is at its flattest. When the lens is at its roundest, rays which diverge from a point only 5 inches in front of the eye are focussed on the retina. The lens is therefore essential for accommodation, but, after its removal for cataract, vision, even for near objects, is rendered possible by the use of convex glasses. A star or a distant gas-lamp is seen as a point of light with rays. Usually this figure, which has given origin to the expression “ star-shaped,” shows three greater rays alternating with three lesser rays. Such an image is not produced by a point of light near to the eye, since it is due to the puckering of the lens when flattened against its ligament. It brings into evidence the three axes on the front of the lens and the three axes which alternate with them on the back, with regard to which the lens-fibres are disposed. As an adaptation of living tissues to optical purposes the eye is above admiration, yet it presents many defects, which an optician corrects in the instruments which he manufactures. A remarkable fact in the physiology of- vision is our uncon- sciousness of the imperfections of its organ. An unusual experiment is needed to bring them to our notice. If we look through a common glass lens uncorrected for unequal refrac- tion of rays of different wave-lengths, we recognize that a bright object is shown with a colour-fringe, yet we take no cognizance of the colour-fringes which surround the images of all bright objects focussed upon our retine. If we think about the matter, we recognize a feeling that blue in a window of stained glass appears farther away than red; but this might well be due to association. Blue glass is chiefly used for the sky. If we look at a bright object through purple glass, we 394 THE BODY AT WORK see it either red with a blue fringe or blue with a red fringe, — according as the eye is focussed for red or for blue. The purple glass having absorbed all intermediate rays, we become aware that we cannot focus the two extreme ends of the spectrum at the same place. Since a greater effort of accom- modation is needed to focus red, we judge that the bright object is nearer to us when it appears red than when it appears blue. Spherical aberration is another fault of the lens. The rays which enter its margin are brought to a focus sooner than those which pass through its centre. This is due to the fact that its surfaces are regularly curved, whereas a glass lens is cor- rected by grinding it flatter towards the margin. This defect is partly corrected by the cornea, which has an ellipsoidal surface, and partly by the greater density of the centre of the lens. Yet it is still necessary for the eye to be “stopped down ”’ by the iris when a near object is looked at, although less light is entering the eye than when it is directed to the horizon—a condition which would lead a photographer to open his iris-diaphragm. Of all the imperfections of the eye which the mind ignores, the most remarkable is the gap in the field of vision, due to the gap in the sensitive layers of the retina, which occurs where the optic nerve enters it—the blind spot. Hold this page of the book 10 inches from the face, keeping the lines of print horizontal. Close the left eye and look at X with the right eye. The black disc disappears, because its image is focussed of on the blind spot. Since the picture on the retina is reversed, it is clear that the optic nerve enters the globe to its inner side, and slightly above its horizontal meridian. But, unless we employ an unusual test, we are quite unconscious of the fact that a definite hole is punched in the picture. The mind fills it in, and the way in which it does so is extremely suggestive. VISION 395 It lies about it—in a downright ingenuous fashion if it is con- fident of credence, in a more subtle way if a simple falsehood is _ likely to be challenged. In place of the black disc make nine conspicuous crosses: ay ee aX >. ae, Sea 4 Hold the paper in such a position that X falls upon the blind spot. It ought to disappear, but the mind assures you that there is a cross at that spot. The mind completes the field. In place of the crosses use noughts and crosses, thus : O X O », ee. Cn». < O X O Now let X fall on the blind spot, and allow the eye to go just a little out of focus. The four marginal crosses draw inwards : O O xX xX O O The mind contracts the field. Still denying the gap, but not having sufficient data from which to invent an object, the fraudulent nature of which would not be found out the instant that the gaze is shifted, the mind lies regarding the position on the paper occupied by surrounding objects. Is it quite fair to the mind to say that it lies about the blind spot? The mind judges sensations in the light of experience. An association of previous sensations teaches me that the wall of the room is not pierced by a round hole a foot in diameter opening into outer darkness. Many sensations 396 THE BODY AT WORK _ have discovered to me the fact that the designs on a wall- paper succeed one another with unbroken regularity. Fixing my gaze on one of them, I cannot by any effort of attention efface the pattern which happens to be focussed on the blind spot. I know that I shall see it the instant that I move the eye. If I let my eye roam until the face of my wife falls on the blind spot, its image disappears. I know its lineaments far better than I know the pattern on the wall-paper, but I cannot fill it into the picture. Her hands are visible, and the work which is resting in her lap, but in a mysterious way the background draws together where the face should be. My mind refuses to pass a false judgment; but it also refuses to see that there is a gap. This exceedingly instructive observation teaches the rela- tivity of sensations. It shows that a sensation has no objec- tive value until judgment has been passed upon it by the mind. The meaning of this we express in figurative language, none other being available. We speak of a new sensation as being compared with sensations previously received—taken - into the picture-gallery of the mind, and placed in its due position amongst the infinitely numerous records which are stored there. If we try to make a nearer approach to cor- relating physical with psychical activity, we say that sensa- tion has no value save that which it acquires from its temporal relation in the sequence of sensations to which attention is directed, and that this value depends upon the relation which similar sensations have possessed in former sequences. There is no gap in binocular vision. An object focussed on the inner (nasal) side of the right eye, where the blind spot is situate, is focussed on the outer (temporal) side of the left eye. The left eye sees the object to which the right eye is blind. Since we have almost invariably used two eyes in the past, experience teaches that there is no gap in the field of vision. Hence the new group of sensations which alleges that there is a gap must be corrected. The field must be filled up in the way which experience shows to be most likely. The retina is a sheet of rods and cones, each of which has a nervous con- nection with the brain proper to itself. The retinal field is associated with the brain-field. But this does not imply that we may think of the mind as having a spatial distribution ie en o ‘ Pes oe <= oe 4 Per oe VISION : 397 in the brain. Pressure on button A or button B in the retina causes bell A’ or bell B’ to ring in the brain, but it does not follow that perception A” or perception B” will be heard in the mind. It will be heard if this is the association established by custom, since mind is the product of experience. But the new sensation is creating precedent as well as being judged by it. Point A in the right retina is associated by experience with point a in the left, and point B with 6. These are termed corresponding points, because they are similarly stimulated in binocular vision. ‘The mind, therefore, judges that it receives the same information from each pair of corresponding points. The position of corresponding points will be understood if the right retina is imagined as put inside the left, precautions being taken to make the yellow spots coincide, and to avoid twisting the retinal cups in taking them out of the eyeballs. Great care is taken to maintain the points in correspondence during the various movements of the two eyeballs. In addition to the four recti muscles which move the eyeball upwards, down- wards, to right and left, two oblique muscles give it the re- quisite amount of rotation. We have learned to give the same value to the impulses from two corresponding points. But under changed conditions the correspondence changes. When a squint develops in childhood, it follows one of two courses ; either the obliquity of one of the eyeballs increases until it looks towards the nose, and its images cease to inter- fere with the images in the dominant eye—they are ignored by the mind—or a fresh correspondence is established between points in the oblique eye and points in the eye which looks straight forward. If we are severely critical, we find, from a study of the form of the eyeball, that it is impossible that the same rods and cones should occupy corresponding points in different positions of focus and with different degrees of con- vergence of the eyeballs. To permit of this the retinal cups would need to change in shape. But again mechanical cor- respondence is of little consequence. In the light of experience the mind judges that points correspond. When we are gazing at a flat surface, the mind judges that corresponding points are giving it similar information. It does not see a flower on a wall-paper twice as bright or twice as red with two eyes as with one. If the eyes are normal the ae or solid objects, the image on one retina is not the same 2 the , image on the other. One eye sees farther round the object on — the one side, the other on the other; and it is just this dis- — parity in the pictures, aided by the feeling that the eyes are converging, that gives the impression of solidity. Correspon- dence of points, on the other hand, is not necessarily sufficient by itself to convince the mind that the pictures presented by the two eyes are identical. When a flat triangle such as this is regarded with the two eyes, its black lines fall on correspond- ing points ; but the figure is associated in the mind with other sensations—sensations of movement and touch. Notwith- standing the identity of the retinal images, the mind tries to see them as disparate. The figure troubles the eyes. At one moment the meeting-point of the three central lines projects forwards, at the next it recedes. That similarity of retinal images counts for something is shown by closing one eye. The uncertainty of shape of the figure is rendered more trouble- some. It changes still more rapidly from convex to concave. When the point seems to be in front of the page, the accommo- dation of the eyes is adjusted for nearness ; when behind the page, for greater distance. But the illusion that the object occupies three dimensions is not dependent upon the sense of contraction of the ciliary muscle. When the paper is moved towards the eye, its centre recedes ; it is left behind until the ciliary muscle has had time to contract. When it is moved away from the eye, it projects until the ciliary muscle has had time to relax. Accommodation follows judgment, not judg- 399 _ the veracity of its newsagents. Disparateness of images, con- vergence of the eyeballs, shifting of accommodation for the various levels of an object in space, should be indisputable evidence of solidity or of hollowness. Conversely, the absence of either factor should be conclusive proof of flatness. But the mind does not trust to isolated sensations ; it looks for — associations of sensations. When the finger hints, “I could touch that sharp point,” it is useless for the eye to aver that there is no point to be touched. If two exactly similar photographs are placed in a stereo- scope, the fact that the eyes are not converged gives to the common picture an appearance of depth, notwithstanding the fact that corresponding points on the two retine are stimu- lated. If the two photographs have been taken, as they should be taken for this purpose, with a double camera, the disparity of the retinal images immensely enhances the impres- sion of solidity. It is impossible to exaggerate the dependence of sensation on judgment. At birth a child commences the long process of education which enables it to associate the sensations derived from its retinal images with the movements which place it in contact with things. It discovers that, when it is necessary to make the eyes converge, the object is near at hand. It also associates the voluntary action of contracting its ciliary muscle with nearness. Unconverged and unaccom- modated eyes come to mean distance. So, too, do indistinct- ness due to absorption by the atmosphere, blueness due to the same cause, a small image on the retina. But there are obvious limits to its power of ascertaining the distance of an object, and therefore, conversely, of its power of estimating size. We have no idea of the size of the retinal image of the sun. Very few people would be prepared to believe that the angle which the sun subtends with the eye barely exceeds half a degree. (The first finger, viewed in profile, at arm’s length, covers one degree of arc.) A disc of paper of the right size, placed at the right distance, looks far too small to represent the sun. The most brilliant of orbs bulks larger than this in our minds. Everyone who for the first time looks at the sun through well-smoked glass, or, better, through a flat-sided 400 _ THE BODY AT WORK vessel filled with ink and water, is astonished that it looks so — small. Nor are we prepared to accept the evidence of a camera that the sun at the zenith does not produce a smaller image on the retina than the sun when rising above the horizon. Yet if a photographic plate is exposed to the rising sun, and again, without changing its focus, to the sun at the zenith, the two images are practically equal. There is a slight difference due to the greater refraction of rays passing tangentially through the atmosphere, but it is so slight as to bear no relation to the difference between our two judgments of size. When the sun is rising behind trees and houses, we compare it with objects which we know to be large and distant ; yet it looks almost as large when rising out of the sea. One of the causes of the illusion is our conviction that the sky is flattened ; and this, again, is due partly to its paler tint—its less substantial blueness—near the horizon, and partly to our impression that it is spread out over a flat earth. When the sun is in what we deem to be the more distant part of the vault of heaven, we judge it to be farther from us, and therefore larger than when it is above us. Yet the last word has not been said in explanation of a pheno- menon which has been studied by mankind since the dawn of science. Helmholtz attributed the apparent greater distance, and consequent greater size, of the sun and moon when near the horizon to the indistinctness of their discs. When its image is so reflected from the zenith as to cause the moon to appear to rest upon the horizon, it does not, he said, increase in size. In answer to Helmholtz’s explanation, it may be objected that, when at midnight he brought the full moon down from the zenith, he did not bring with her the conditions of light and colour by which she is customarily surrounded when floating on the horizon. If, when watching the moon which has just risen, vast in diameter, out of the sea, one interposes between it and the eye a sheet of paper in which a small hole has been made, and looks at the moon with one eye through the hole, it instantly shrinks to the size which it appears to have at the zenith. It is not even necessary to blot out the whole of its trail of light on the sea. At the same time, it appears to retreat to a great distance. This shows how complicated are the associations upon which judgments of size and distance are based, and to how small an extent are de termi | LAER obs rvation is aes surprising if made one or two nights full 1 moon, when twilight is already dim at moon-rise. Our estimate of the distance away from us of an object on t is based upon the time and effort which experience “tells us we should need to spend in reaching it. The untried _ appears shorter than the tried. Anyone who compares his feeling of the number of yards he would have to climb up a pole reaching to the zenith with his feeling of the number of steps he would need to take to reach the horizon will recognize that the horizon appears to him to be the farther away. ee vat Fig. 36.—A SYMMETRICAL ARCH, DIVIDED BY A VERTICAL LINE, A, WHICH PASSES THROUGH Ivs APEX. In representing a solid object an artist conveys the idea that light is falling obliquely upon it. One side of the object, therefore, is more strongly illuminated than the other. By depth and gradation of shade he indicates.the extent to which the thing projects forwards, if solid, or falls back, if hollow, He makes the margin of a ball hazy, in the expectation that the spectator will look at the spot nearest to him—an artifice which he may easily press too far, since the eyes wander rest- lessly over a flat surface. In representing distance he is dependent upon giving to the various objects in his picture sizes equivalent to the sizes of their images on the retina, making them brighter or paler and more or less distinct. Yet he cannot hope to simulate the convincing evidence of distance which is afforded by our sense of the degree of convergence of 26 402 THE BODY AT WORK our eyes. Hence, as Francis Bacon pointed out, a picture — appears more real when one eye is closed than when both are © open. Its middle distance at once falls back. . > Innumerable are the illustrations which may be given of errors of sensory judgment, but none are more striking than the various figures which may be drawn with converging or diverging lines. The mind under-estimates acute and over- estimates obtuse angles. It is impossible to convince oneself that in Fig. 36 the line A bisects asymmetrical arch. Equally difficult is it to believe that in Fig. 37 the line with diverging terminal segments and the line with converging terminal seg- ments are of exactly equal length. In the Ruskin Museum ier ee Fie. 37.—Two HORIZONTAL LINES OF EQUAL LENGTH—THE ONE WITH DIVERGING, THE OTHER WITH CONVERGING, TERMINAL LINES. at Sheffield there is a sketch by the master of the facade of a church which shows a vertical tower to one side of a triangular pediment, or, rather, this is what the sketch was meant to show, and does show, when measured on an architect’s table. In effect the tower appears to be leaning towards the pediment. _ Errors of judgment of this type have been attributed to the curvature of the lines of a rectilinear image on the retina, the mind judging the distance between two points by the length of the chord, and not the length of the are which joins them. This is very simply illustrated by the example of the apparently greater length of a filled space than of a vacant one. A B C A B looks longer than B C. If A BC be represented as a curved. line, the arc A B will, of course, be longer than the chord B C. But it is not safe to suppose that the mind com- pares the length of an arc with the length of a chord. Judg- ment is based upon experience, and probably the illusion is due to more subtle causes than the curvature of the retina. Dyce ane i ee a eT et ware PERT Pe Sieh: fal fons 6 mae fe aS re . ee = me hc ee ee 2 Ane et a peat ee VISION 403 _ The mind does not look at the retina. If it did, it would find _ the reversal of the picture the least of the inaccuracies which it had to correct. It would find it very difficult, for example, _ to superpose in its stereoscope the photographs of a vertical tower taken simultaneously by the right eye and the left. The curved images on the retina of the vertical lines which define the angles of the tower, as seen with one eye, could not be made to correspond with the images focussed by the other eye. The Greeks felt this when they settled the form of a column. The canon of the swelling entasis and increasing taper above it did not destroy the appearance of uniform thickness which the shaft presented. It gave to the eye just the slight help which it needs to enable it to picture the shaft as of the same thickness from base to capital. 26—2 CHAPTER XIV HEARING Tue ear, like the eye, records amplitude of vibration ; loudness. It also records rapidity of vibration, musical pitch, which corre- sponds with colour. But it seems to have a more difficult task than the eye, since it has to analyse, or at any rate has to transmit information regarding the form of compound vibra- tions. ‘The meanings of these distinctions may be illustrated by reference to a tracing on the cylinder of a phonograph. A needle attached to the posterior surface of the thin metal plate against which one speaks scratches the surface of a rotating cylinder of hardened wax. Examined with a lens, the record is seen to be an irregularly changing line. The depth of the marks is a measure of loudness. Their varying number in a given time indicates the changing pitch of the voice which produced them. Their form is a record of the quality of its tone. The work of the ear, so far as it consists in the estimation of the amplitude and rapidity of pulsations of sound, is easy to describe, but the acoustics of form are complicated. Light is transmitted as vibrations of ether. They are transverse to the direction in which the light is travelling. Sound cannot travel through a vacuum, since it is dependent upon displacements of material particles. The particles move forwards and backwards in the direction in which sound is progressing. Sound is a sequence of pulsations, alternate con- densations and rarefactions of the media which conduct it. Their particles are first pressed together, and then rebound to positions farther apart. A sequence of to-and-fro movements, each smoothly continuous throughout the whole duration of a pulsation, would produce a pure musical tone. Tuning-forks carefully bowed settle down after a few seconds into unbroken oscillations, which convey to the air the to-and-fro movements of pure tones. Such tones vary in nothing but loudness and pitch. If their pulsations are slow, we speak of the pitch as “low”; if they are rapid, we say that their pitch is high. 404 ; HEARING ; 405 - But if the sound produced by tuning-forks (and low-toned _ stopped organ-pipes) be omitted from the list, no pure tones - reach our ears. The notes of flutes, fiddles, trumpets, pianos, have each a certain “ quality’ characteristic of the instru- ment. Even in a violin the G string has not the same timbre as the D string. Owing to the elasticity of the substances which originate and of the substances which transmit sound. its pulsations are not simple to-and-fro movements, uninter- rupted from beginning to end. Each pulsation is partially broken at intervals ; and the quality of the sound depends upon the number and relative acccntuation of these partial inter- ruptions. Sound travels through air at the rate of 1,100 feet per second. This figure, divided by the number of vibrations per second of a tone, gives the wave-length in air of a tone of that particular pitch. For example, the middle C has a vibratory rate of 256. Its wave-length is, therefore, somewhat over 4 feet. The lowest tone of an organ has a wave-length of 37 feet ; its highest of 3} inches. These figures give no infor- mation, however, regarding the movement of the particles which pass on the sound. When air is transmitting a note— say the middle C—its separate molecules do not move through a distance of 4 feet. Each molecule moves but a short distance, varying with the loudness of the tone; but the “wave” of crowding runs straight forward from the piano-string to the ear, the molecules at the end of each stage of 4 feet taking on a backward movement, so that the crowding, so far as the mole- cules of that particular section are concerned, returns to its starting-point. Between the piano-string and the ear there is a crowding and forward movement at 0, 4, 8,12... feet; a spreading and backward movement at 2, 6, 10, 14... feet. Most illustrations which are intended to aid the mind in forming a definite picture of the transmission of sound are liable to be misinterpreted, because they translate rectilinear movements into waves. They represent the movements of the string, and not the movements of the molecules of air between the string and the ear; but with the aid of the imagination one may picture the positions of the particles in this path. The pulse, we will suppose, has just reached the limit of 12 feet. Half-way from its 8-foot halting place the molecules are again crowded, althotigh not so densely. One-third of the distance from the same point there again appears a tendency to es the two ee WAVES, divided into fou these intervals are other points at which the : closed together, the distances from a nodal point ¢ deper upon the number of waves involved, and, speaking general growing less marked as the number increases. Such are t very complex pulsatile movements which reach the ear, Every musical sound produced by a piano, a violin, or other! : instrument, is compounded of a fundamental or prime tone, — and overtones, partial tones, or harmonics. The following — table shows the more important partial tones which accompany © the prime tone when the middle ( on a pianoforte is struck : Note sl areca Interval. Ratio, | Sete gui 2,048 7th ’ 8 \ Super-second 7 B''b 1,792 6th Sabdninordhied a G!! 1,536 5th Minor third = Ell 1,280 Ath 5 Major third 7 Cll 1,024 ) 8rd > Fourth = G' 768 < Qnd 38 r Fifth Qo 2 C! 512 Ast A ° . Octave a C 256 , - HEARING | | 407 The Stall of a atin note depends upon the number and relative loudness of its overtones. When several notes are sounded simultaneously, they blend into a chord or har- mony, provided the intervals which separate them are equal to the intervals which separate the simpler overtones. Each of the notes yields overtones. The tones blend into a concord. Their partials are in unison. The variations in air-pressure of the compound tone are strictly periodic. If the ratios of the frequencies of its constituent notes are simple the product is a rich, full sound, such as a common chord. At least one other character of the pulsations of sound must be taken into consideration if we wish to picture the nature of the force to which the ear responds. Tones which reach it from several instruments simultaneously are not necessarily in unison, or even in harmony. The overtones of a single note sounded on a piano or violin—the statement does not hold good for bells, nor is it strictly true of flutes or horns—must necessarily bear a simple proportional relation to their prime tone. They divide the grand pulsation into fractions ‘ with- out a remainder.” But the vibrations of two tuning-forks which are slightly out of unison interfere one with the other at regular intervals. They produce “ beats.” Everyone is familiar with the curious effect which is produced upon the eye when one row of railings is seen through another, or one expanse of wire-netting behind another. Sets of lines which occupy nearly the same positions in the line of sight combine to make a large pattern, which overlies the smaller pattern of the rails or netting. The same thing happens with sounds which coin- cide at considerable intervals, although in the case of sounds interference is as marked as reinforcement. If whilst a tuning- fork yielding 101 vibrations per second is singing another of 100 vibrations is brought into play, the vibrations of the second fork are superposed on those of the first. At a certain moment the forward movement of molecules of air induced by the first fork is reinforced by a forward push from the second. But half a second after this coincidence of phase an opposite result is produced—504 vibrations of No. 1 have passed, but only 50 of No. 2. No. 2 is going backwards (inwards), whilst No. 1 is moving forwards (outwards). The same molecules are im- pelled backwards by No. 2 and forwards by No. 1. The result 408 THE BODY AT WORK is a pause. The compound sound produced by the two forks reaches the ear in throbs. If the forks were vibrating at the rates of 101 and 99, there would be two pauses and two beats in every second ; if at the rate of 202 and 198, four. The number of beats per second equals the difference in frequency of vibration of the tones. A pianoforte tuner does his work best if he has a musical ear, yet he may discharge his duties with competence without one. Having struck a note, he sounds its octave, holding both keys down, and listens for the beat. If the first note gave no beat with his tuning-fork, the second is in tune when it likewise gives no beat with the first. We have met a tuner who did his work in this way ; but it must be admitted that his tempering of the intervals of the octave with which he commenced, and consequently of the other octaves above and below it, left something to be desired. The result might have been satisfactory had he been provided with twelve tuning-forks. The question as to whether beats, when sufficiently rapid, blend into a tone has been much discussed, without a decision. Probably they do not. The complementary question as to the cause of dissonance is also not completely closed. Two notes harmonize, as we have seen, when the ratio of their frequencies is a simple fraction. Musicians are not quite agreed as to the level of numerical complexity at which a compound tone first produces a feeling of discomfort. A good deal depends upon its position in the scale and the instruments which are combining to produce it. A minor third (&) is on the safe side. This is the first chord in our list of intervals in which a beat can be detected. Slow beats, however, do not distress us. It is the rapid beats of conflicting overtones which give a harsh, rough character to a compound note. The level at which a line is drawn between harmony and dissonance seems to depend to a considerable extent upon musical educa- tion, using the term in its widest sense. In primitive music— Hungarian, Scotch, Welsh—intricate minor chords predomi- nate. The minute subdivision of the octave in Indian music is quite incomprehensible to a European ear. Musical cultiva- tion tends to eliminate complex fractions. It is, however, to be noted that the history of Western music also shows the influence of an opposite tendency. Later generations have 409 _ admitted as harmonies combinations which earlier generations could not tolerate. Pitch, quality, harmony, and dissonance are distinguished by the human ear. These are the attributes of musical or periodic sounds. In a separate class must be included noises of all kinds, termed in acoustics “ aperiodic,’”’ because the vibrations which cause them are not rhythmic. The teeth of a policeman’s rattle may click a hundred times a second, but it does not make music. Even with a rapidity of interruption greater than this (at least 500 times per second) a succession of noises fails to blend into a smooth, continuous sound. The ear recognizes the loudness, duration, and even to a very high frequency the repetition of unmusical sounds. The ear as a sense-organ can be followed down the zoological scale to jelly-fish. In its primitive form it is a chamber lined with epithelial cells bearing hairs, containing an otolith, or ear-stone. Otoliths are rounded calcareous masses which play an important part in the ears of all animals up to fishes. Even in man they are found in the more subdivided form of otoconia. Contact of the otoliths with the sensory hairs originates impulses in the nerves with which primitive ears are abundantly provided. Advisedly we use the word “ ear ”’ in place of “auditory organ.” In all animals this organ affords information of a double nature—movement of the external medium in which the animal lives, and movements of the animal in the medium. When the animal moves, its sensory hairs are displaced with regard to the otolith ; when the water in which it is swimming pulsates, its otoliths are shaken against the sensory hairs. Displacements of the animal and agitations of the water produce similar effects. The ear in this stage is an organ of touch. It might well be ques- tioned whether an animal fitted with a piece of sensory appar- atus of this kind is endowed with a sense which we may properly, after reflecting upon our own sensations, term “hearing.” It is, however, stated that certain transparent crustaceans, in which the functioning of the ear-organs may be watched through a lens, show in these organs hairs of varying length which vibrate to tones of different frequency. This observation apart, it might be doubted whether fishes hear, if we mean by the word “ hearing ” the recognition and 410 THE BODY AT WORK discrimination of tones of high frequency—musical tones. Their ears serve equally to inform them of the changes in position of their heads and of the tremblings of the sea. The shocks transmitted through the sea are near akin to the slower vibrations of sound, if the fishermen of the Mediter- ranean are justified in their practice of beating a wooden clapper which rests upon the seat of the boat as they row backwards and forwards in front of a curved net. They believe that the fish are frightened by the noise ; but it matters little whether we describe the fish as hearing a noise, or as feeling the percussions of the clapper conducted through the water. To the more rapid vibrations of the clapper, the fish are probably insensitive. The cochlea, which we have every reason for regarding as the organ by which sound is analysed, is not possessed by fishes. It makes its first appearance in reptiles. Birds, it is evident, are able to distinguish musical tones. Their cochlee are very short, and are destitute of “rods of Corti.” For a moment this appears surprising, but it must be remembered that the range of tones which any bird discriminates is very short, however nicely it may value the notes within its range. In mammals the ear is clearly divided into three parts, to which the three functions which have grown out of the specialization of the sense of touch are allocated. (1) The semicircular canals are concerned with the sense of orientation. (2) The utricle and saccule rever- berate to noise—the rumbling of trains, the boom of guns, the beats of dissonant musical tones. We do not know how to classify the agitations of the atmosphere which surrounds us and of the earth on which we stand, nor can we point with any certainty to the groups of stimuli which for us have taken the place of the grinding of stones on the beach and slapping of rocks by waves. (3) The organ of Corti in the cochlea discriminates and analyses musical sounds. To these three sense-organs, which are situate in the inner ear, certain structures are accessory. The concha, which enables a horse or a cat to collect sound and to localize its source, is in ourselves merely an ornament to the side of the head. _ The external meatus is a curved tube, about an inch long. Frequently a tuft of hairs guards its entrance. The wax | by its wall serves to attach particles of dust, and to deter insects from entering the tube. The air at the end of it is at a uniform temperature. It is closed by the membrana tympani, or drum. This membrane receives the vibrations of ee © esis = ets ‘ . GIGRIANY prsece cay 08 Fig. 38.—THE EXTERNAL, MIDDLE, AND INTERNAL EAR OF THE LEFT SIDE. From right to left, the figure shows the concha and,lobule of the ear in profile; the external meatus (abbreviated) ; the drum, divided vertically, its posterior half visible ; the hammer- bone, with the tip of its long arm attached to the drum, an arrow indicating the point of attachment and line of action of’ the tensor tympani muscle; the anvil attached by a ligament to the bony wall of the middle ear; the stirrup, with its foot-plate almost filling the oval window ; the labyrinth, with the three semicircular canals above, and the scala vestibuli below. ‘The curled black line shows the situation of the scala media, or ductus cochlese (which contains the organ of Corti). Pulsations of sound which move the mem- brana tympani are transmitted by the three bones to the oval window. They shake the perilymph, producing waves which travel along the scala vestibuli to the apex of the cochlea, whence they return by the scala tympani to the round window (if they do not take a shorter course through the ductus cochlese). The Eustachian tube opens out of the lower part of the middle ear. sound ; and, in order that it may collect them with absolute impartiality, it is in every respect the opposite in shape and structure to the top of a drum. The stretched parchment which covers a drum is flat. Its tension is uniform in all its 412 THE BODY AT WORK parts. Movements have the greatest amplitude at the centre. Every precaution is taken to insure its emitting, with as little confusion as may be, the particular note to which it is tuned. The drum of the ear is shaped like the mouth of a trumpet, depressed to a point, but convex from this point outwards. Its elastic fibres, which are partly radial, partly circular, are at many different tensions. Its deepest part, to which the long arm of the hammer-bone is attached, is not its centre. The “middle ear” is an irregular cavity communicating with the pharynx by the Eustachian tube. It is filled with air at the same pressure as the atmosphere. Except during the act of swallowing, when it is at first shut tightly and then — opened, the pharyngeal end of the Eustachian tube is gently closed. When one is dropped in a lift rapidly down the shaft of a mine, the difference in pressure between the external air and. the air in the middle ear stretches the drum to such an extent that deafness to low tones is produced. Conversation becomes inaudible. The deafness is remedied by swallowing saliva, and thus opening the end of the Eustachian tube. The commonest cause of permanent deafness is inflammation followed by thickening of the mucous membrane of the lower end of the Kustachian tube, with its consequent closure, due to frequent sore-throats. The air in the middle ear is slowly absorbed. It needs to be constantly renewed through the Eustachian tube. On the inner wall of the middle ear are two small apertures —the oval window and the round window. Both are closed with membrane. Into the oval window is fitted the sole-plate of the stirrup-bone. Three bones—hammer, anvil, and stirrup —combine in transferring the movements of the membrana tympani to the oval window. They constitute a jointed lever, which swings about an axis passing through the ligament of the anvil (Fig. 38), the excursions of the long arm of the hammer being reduced in amplitude by one-third at the stirrup-plate. As the oval window has only one-twentieth of the area of the drum, the movements of the latter are trans- mitted with concentrated force. Two points in the mechanism of these bones may be specially noticed : (1) The head of the hammer is free to rotate in the cavity of the anvil, checked by a cog. Every inward movement of the drum is faithfully trans- HEARING 413 mitted to the oval window ; but when the drum moves out- _ wards, the hammer does not necessarily carry the anvil with it. (2) A muscle—tensor tympani—is inserted near the elbow of the long arm of the hammer. When high notes are listened to its contraction tightens the drum, rendering it more respon- sive to rapid vibrations. It has a tonic action, but it does not make any special contraction for low notes. Behind the two windows, within the solid bone, is the inner ear, which our ancestors very aptly termed a “labyrinth.” It is filled with fluid—perilymph—which is shaken by every movement of the stirrup-plate. Since water is incompressible, no waves could be raised in the perilymph were there no second aperture. Every vibration conveyed by the stirrup- plate after passing through the labyrinth ends as a vibration of the membrane which closes the round window. Nowhere does perilymph come in contact with auditory cells. All the endings of the nerve of hearing are contained within a membranous labyrinth which lies within the bony cavities. The way in which the waves of the perilymph are dispersed over the surface of this closed sac can be inferred from the diagram (Fig. 38). They sweep round the utricle and saccule, are lost in the narrow spaces which surround the semicircular canals, run up the scala vestibuli of the cochlea. The course of the waves which traverse the cochlea is of especial interest in connection with the physiology of hearing. The cochlea—snail-shell—is a spiral tunnel of three turns, in hard bone, about an inch in length. A shelf of bone— lamina spiralis—projects into the tunnel on its convex side. From the free margin of this spiral lamina two membranes extend to the outer wall of the tunnel—one firm, containing straight, stiff, and probably elastic fibres which radiate out- wards (the basilar membrane) ; the other an extremely delicate film of connective tissue. The tunnel is thus divided into three compartments, known as the scala vestibuli, scala media, scala tympani. The scala media belongs to the membranous labyrinth. Waves transmitted through perilymph pass, as we have already explained, up the scala vestibuli. At the apex of the cochlea the two scale are in communication ; but the aperture is small, and it is unlikely that waves reach the lower passage from the upper through this opening. ‘They pass 414 THE BODY AT WORK through the thin membrane which roofs the scala media, shake its endolymph, and reach the lower passage through the basilar membrane. It is noteworthy that, since the round window at the lower end of the scala tympani is, with the exception of the oval window, the only opening of the bony labyrinth, all waves transmitted through the oval window must travel part of the way or all the way up and down the cochlea. The organ of Corti is spread out on the basilar membrane. It is an epithelial structure of extreme regularity and uni- — ——=t fg Ae SS P= Oe — \ fj com para SS Vines Va Fig. 39.—A SECTION THROUGH THE AXIS OF THE COLUMN OF THE COOHLEA. The spiral sheet of nerve-fibres which supplies the organ of Corti is cut in eight places. If the bundle to the lowest coil of the shell (on the left side of the diagram) is followed, it will be seen to bear ganglion cells where it enters the bony spiral lamina. This lamina divides the tube into two canals—scala vestibuli above, scala tympani below. From the edge of the lamina the membrane of Corti stretches to the outer wall. Above the organ of Corti is the membrana tectoria, and above this a very thin membrane which cuts off the ductus cochlee from the scala vestibuli. formity. Near to the edge by which the basilar membrane is attached to the spiral lamina rests a double row of rods of Corti, stiff pillars which lean one towards the other, over the tunnel of Corti, the convex head of the outer rod fitting into a concavity in the head of the inner one; in some places one outer rod fits against two inner rods, as the latter are rather the more numerous. On the inner side of the inner rod is seen, in transverse sections a single plump cell filled with cloudy protoplasm, and bearing on its free surface a tuft of very short | HEARING 415 hairs. On the outer side of the outer rod are three or four hair-cells, each with a cloudy outer segment containing the nucleus, a granular middle segment, and a stiffish stalk, which attaches it to the basilar membrane. Between the hair-cells are supporting cells, thicker below, tapering above, containing ‘in their substance a firm fibre. Still farther to the outer side are epithelial cells, of no special interest. The purpose of the rods of Corti and the supporting cells is to give attachment and support to a reticulated membrane of exquisite delicacy, through the oblong apertures of which the hairs of the hair- cells project into the endolymph. The spiral lamina is )) © U, 4) lo |O| Oo eV cor A Yr~PakhrDV%-sel-rtoe OLY ~~ > ~ eh, 22 Qe # lo 1O"7_2 BAS eN a SS et A = NS q\ Fig. 40.—ORGAN OF CORTI. The spiral lamina, on the left of the drawing, gives attachment to the membrane of Corti, which stretches to the opposite wall. Below the membrane is a bloodvessel which runs its whole length beneath the tunnel of Corti. The tunnel is formed by pillars—the inner on the left, the outer on the right—which meet above it. On the left of the inner pillar is a hair-cell ; to the left of this a nerve-cell with two nuclei. To the right of the outer pillar is a space ; to the right of this four hair-cells alternating with four supporting cells, which hold up the reticulated membrane through apertures in which the tufts of hairs project. Three nerve-fibres are seen in the spiral lamina ; they cross the tunnel to ramify between the rows of outer hair-cells. The lamina tectoria rests upon the tufts of hairs. traversed by a vast number of fibres of the auditory nerve, which, losing their medullary sheaths, pass across the tunnel of Corti as naked axons, to end amongst the hair-cells. Above the organ of Corti, attached by its edge to the spiral lamina, is a thick, gelatinous, fibrillated structure—membrana tectoria— which rests as a coverlet on the surface of the organ. It has been supposed that it serves to damp the vibrations of the hairs after they have been set in motion by the waves passing across the scala media ; but it not impossibly plays a more active part in hearing than this. The organ of Corti is, beyond doubt, the apparatus which analyses sounds; but the problem of the way in which it Me tu) eee 416 THE BODY AT WORK responds to tones of different pitch, or analyses compound tones, is not as yet even approximately solved. To escape the acoustic difficulties which have to be faced by anyone who endeavours to expound the theory of the cochlea as a piece of analytical apparatus, various suggestions as to the possibility of an action en masse have been advanced. For example, the basilar membrane has been compared to a telephone-plate which takes up vibrations and transmits them through the auditory nerve to the brain. But if the organ of Corti be the transmitter, there is no ear in the brain to analyse the vibra- tions given out by a receiving telephone-plate ; and without a receiving plate and a listening ear a telephone is purposeless. According to this hypothesis, the basilar membrane vibrates as a whole, moving the hair-cells in various “patterns ”’; the pres- sure of the hairs against the tectorial membrane causing irrita- tion of the cells which bear them, and hence producing stimula- tion of various groups of nerves. Other pattern theories are somewhat similar. But it is obvious that all hypotheses of the vibration of the whole of the basilar membrane, or of large parts of it, simultaneously, leave to the mind the responsibility of reading the pattern which the impulses generated in the organ of Corti make in the brain. It is conceivable that every fraction of a semitone which a musician can discriminate, and every combination of tones which he can analyse, is trans- mitted to the brain by a large number of co-operating nerve- impulses ; but such a theory involves a complexity of mental associations difficult to contemplate. According to the general principles enunciated in this book, analysis of stimuli is the function of sense-organs. It cannot in all cases be compared with the analysis effected in a physical laboratory ; nor is this necessary ; but it must be carried so far that nerve-impulses which have no specific qualities apart from their source shall give rise to effects in consciousness which have no basis other than the topographical distribution of the said impulses in the brain. There may be sensory impulses of different orders; there may be in the brain psycho- physical substances which react to impulses of various orders in various ways; but until we have some hint of the existence of specific impulses and specific psycho-physical substances, we are not justified in postulating their existence a 4 eae ae : HEARING 417 7 | simply in order that we may escape from physiological em- _ barrassments. The organ of Corti has in the highest degree the appearance of a piece of apparatus for the analysis of sound. If the basilar membrane, with the cells which rest upon it, be cut out and laid flat, the suggestion of some kind of instrument is very strong. It is a long narrow ribbon, narrowest at the bottom of the spiral, increasing to about twice the width at the apex. It is crossed by radiating fibres, presumably elastic. The cells which rest upon it carry vibrating hairs, and are supplied with nerves. The rods of Corti hold up the reticulated membrane, which keeps the hair-cells in place. It is not to be wondered at that when its structure was first discovered it was thought that the problem of the analysis of musical tones was solved. If two pianos in perfect tune are in the same room, when one is played the corresponding wires of the other twang. Anyone who sings into a piano, whilst the loud pedal raises the dampers, feels an increased fulness in his voice. This is the familiar phenomenon of resonance. Why should not the fibres of the basilar membrane resonate to the tones conveyed to the ear—the shorter ones at the base of the cochlea to high tones, the longer ones at the apex to low tones ? This is the order in which we should expect the pulsations of sound which ascend the scala vestibuli to be taken up—the more rapid near its commencement, the less rapid farther up it. But an explanation of the physics of the selection of vibrations of different frequencies by different sets of the elements which make up the organ of Corti, if such selec- tion occurs, is still to seek. In the first place, the fibres of the basilar membrane are so exceedingly short. What could a fibre less than 0-5 millimetre in length make of the vibrations of a 36-foot organ-pipe? Even if this objection be waived, as certain eminent physicists hold that it may be, there is not a sufficient difference in length between the longest and the shortest fibres to account for the great range of tones which we are able to discriminate ; nor is there any evidence that some fibres are more tightly stretched than others. A further consideration which tempts physiologists to look upon the organ of Corti (including the basilar membrane) as a series of resonators is the somewhat remarkable agreement 27 418 THE BODY AT WORK between the number of separate pieces of apparatus of which — it appears to be composed and the number of different musical — sounds which, if it were a series of resonators, it might be called upon to discriminate. The squeak given by a bat at each turn in its flight has a pitch of about 11,000 vibrations to the second—the sixth E above the middle C (Tyndall). In a group of persons listening for the squeak there are usually some who cannot hear it. Above this the range of hearing is very variable. The sudden- ness of transition from perfect hearing to total want of per- ception makes experiments with small pipes or with a siren somewhat amusing, when a number of persons are tested at the same time. One complains that the note is intolerably loud and shrill, whilst others assert that there is perfect silence. Thirty- three thousand vibrations is usually regarded as the upper limit for the human ear, but certain physiologists place it at 40,000, or even higher. The upper limit is of little consequence, since there is very little power of discriminating rapidities above the highest note used in music—the piccolo stop of the organ, with a pitch of 4,096. It is possible that a sound with a lower frequence than 27 (the contra-bassoon) may be heard as a tone—16 according to certain writers; but again our power of discriminating very low notes is small. Over a certain range a skilled musician can tell that a note is out of tune when it is one sixty-fourth of a semitone higher or lower than it ought to be. If we assume that by allowing equal sensitiveness for a range of seven octaves, the excess of the allowance over the actual sensitiveness towards either end of this stretch would compensate for the comparatively few distinctions which the ear can make either below or above it— 64x 12x7=5,376. A much higher estimate, based upon observations which seem to show that the ear can distinguish sounds less than one sixty-fourth of a semitone apart, places the total number at 11,000. On the assumption that one piece of apparatus is tuned to resonate for every distinguishable sound, between 5,000 and 11,000 pieces of apparatus would be required. Taking one of Corti’s arches as the centre-piece of the resonator, although the rods are certainly not vibratile structures, we find the number to be 3,848 (the number of the outer rods) ; HEARING 419 if either rod with a hair-cell, or hair-cells, is the analytical - element, 9,438. Counting gives 3,487 inner, 11,700 outer, hair-cells. The fibres of the basilar membrane are estimated at 24,000 ; the fibres of the cochlear nerve at 14,000. It will be understood that the counting of structures as minute as these yields results which cannot be more than approximately accurate. Helmholtz, assuming that each are of Corti indi- cates an analytical element, accounted for the apparent deficiency in their number by assuming that a tone of which the pitch fell between two arches set both in sympathetic vibra- tion, the arch which was nearest in pitch to the tone vibrating the more strongly. In this way he anticipated an objection which has often been brought against his theory of a long series of resonators. In opposition to Helmholtz’s theory it is pointed out that when a violinist runs his finger up a bowed string, the pitch rises with perfect smoothness ; it does not bump along from resonator to resonator. LEspecially in the case of very high tones given out by a siren, it is urged that at the rare intervals at which a resonator in the ear is tuned for the tone which the siren is emitting it should sound much louder than when the tone falls midway between two resonators. But the whole question of the nature of the response of the analytical elements is too obscure at present for the discussion of points so nice as this. Many who think that Helmholtz’s theory of resonators is based upon principles of physics and of physiology which must be regarded as the starting-points of any explanation of the analysis of sounds by the ear and the mind, hold that it goes too far in searching for a separate resonator for every dis- tinguishable tone. The cochlea, as we have already said, does not offer anything like so extensive a choice as this, if regard be had to the tension or length of its elements, and not to their numbers. Those who accept it as an axiom that the cochlea contains a series of responding instruments—but a series far more limited in range than the gamut of our sound-percep- tions—seek to discover in musical tones qualities which unite them in groups. Just as in the case of colour-sensations they recognize four (or six) elementary qualities which excite four (or six) pieces of responding apparatus, so also in the case of hearing they seek for a limited number of tone-qualities and 27—2 “ 420 THE BODY AT WORK a correspondingly limited number of elementary sensations. The ideal of those who take this view is an octave of qualities and of elementary sensations sounded in the middle of the scale when x nerve-endings are stimulated, as the octave above when 2x nerves respond, the octave below with st Such a conception seems to guide thought round insurmountable barriers. There is, however, a risk of making too much of the periodic intervals, because they take so important a place in music. At one side of the gap which sound bridges between the individual and his environment is an elastic body shaking at any possible rate within the range of hearing. At the other side of the gap is the ear. If, having arranged several thou- sands of stones along the side of the road in order of size, I were to state, picking up No. 512, “‘ This is the fundamental of which No. 1,024 is the octave,’ answer would be made to me: “It may be that the larger could be broken into halves, each as heavy as the smaller stone ; but I recognize no differ- — ence between the stones in shape, colour, or hardness.” A vibrating string divides into equal segments, each of which vibrates within the vibrations of the whole string, sounding the octave. We recognize a similarity in quality between tones and their octaves because we are accustomed to hear the octave, the most prominent of overtones, in all musical sounds. Hence, from association, it has become more difficult to dis- tinguish a note from its octave than it is to distinguish it from its fifth ; but it does not follow that the effect of 1,024 vibrations upon the sensory cells more nearly resembles the effect of 512 than does that of 768. But at this point we are compelled to construct some hypothesis as to the way in which the vibra- tions affect the sensory cells. The protoplasm of the cells is not directly sensitive to them. We can account for the generation of impulses in the nerve connected with a par- ticular cell, or group of cells, only on the supposition that a resonating mechanism which responds to vibrations of a certain frequency shakes the cell. Even then it seems necessary to suppose that there is an accessory mechanism which disturbs the cell protoplasm sufficiently to render the shake effective, probably the hairs rubbing against the tectorial membrane. Anatomical study gives us no confidence in the theory of the HEARING — 421 existence of several thousands of resonators tuned to as many notes of different pitch. It remains for the physicists to say whether or not we may picture one of these minute resonators as responding to a given note in 10 separate octaves, another in 9... another in only 1. The physicists, on their part, may very properly ask the anatomists to point out the resonators, and even to reproduce them in models of dimensions which allow of experimental investigation. It is generally agreed that the sensation of a chord is com- pounded of the sensations to which each of its constituent tones gives rise, and that our power of analysing the com- pound is a question of attention. A musician can direct his attention to either sensation at will. It is not equally certain that a person who has no knowledge of music can do the same. Familiarity with musical instruments gives us so exact a know- ledge of the way in which compound tones are produced that it becomes a difficult matter to decide whether, when we say that we can pick out the E or the G of the common chord, it means that we can hear it as distinct from C and C’, or whether it means that, knowing the constitution of the chord, we think about the E or the G when we hear the compound tone, to the exclusion of its other constituents. Then, again, the several strings which we try to strike simultaneously do not actually “toe the line.” Their vibrations are not in the same phase, even though the strings be in absolute tune. Dis- crepancy of phase may favour the singling out of the several constituents of the chord. There we touch upon a problem which we passed over in silence when attempting to give an idea of the nature of the pulsations which reach the ear. We then (p. 405) described the partial pulsations which are super- imposed upon the main pulsation as if they necessarily started simultaneously with it. We assumed that the phase difference of the partials was zero. But it is clear that differences of phase of its constituent tones may produce an almost infinite number of variations in the form of a compound “ wave ”’ of sound. Is the ear variously affected by different forms of wave ? Does difference of phase result in difference of sensa- tion ? In broad terms, the answer to this question must be in the negative ; although it can be shown that in certain cases a change in phase of the several constituents of a compound ~~. | a vr Fe Ie, =a 2 Se ee ee a ee —_ « “ . me + 429 ‘THE BODY AT WORK tone, without any alteration in their number or their loudness, — makes a change in its acoustic quality. Any attempt to corre- — late physical changes—the movements of air in the outer ear —with the effects which they may be supposed to have upon the organ of Corti must take into account this wide range of — variation of wave-form. We have called attention to the diffi- culties which it introduces ; but have no hope of indicating the way in which they may be overcome. ; Nothing connected with the physiology of the sense of hear- ing is more remarkable than its capacity for education. The cochlea of one human being is as extensive and as elaborate in structure as that of another, yet some men can make an infinitely more refined use of it as an analytical apparatus than can others. A native of the Torres Straits cannot distinguish as two separate notes sounds which are less than a semitone apart. Sir Michael Costa could distinguish sounds into the sixty-fourth parts of semitones. The cochlea of a cat is not less elaborate than that of a man, yet Man’s mental life is based upon the analysis of auditory sensations. His supreme advance in the animal scale has depended upon the invention of language, by means of which he communicates and receives information, thus rendering experience eternal, notwithstand- ing the transience of the individuals who acquire and transmit it. An animal is born, finds out, dies. A man starts with the wisdom of the race beneath his feet. Hearing has a nebulous origin in sensations of movement or displacement. The connection between the two special senses —the sense of orientation and the sense of hearing, properly so-called—remains always intimate. David danced before the Ark of the Lord. All people, savage and civilized, associate music with movement. High in the animal scale appears the sense-organ which enables its possessor to discriminate musical tones. By its use Man has developed with great rapidity—as secular time is reckoned—an intelligence which removes him from all other animals a planet’s space. The sounding of his organ of Corti by pure tones and combinations of pure tones gives him extreme pleasure, although it in no way ministers to his intelligence. Yet there is in the enjoyment of music a quality of pleasure which makes it near akin to the satisfac- tion which we experience in exercising the intellect. ; sae a eo Sr aloe Be 5h > <5 - Reina bo eee ee PERE h oe Aad oid 3 CHAPTER XV SKIN-SENSATIONS THE senses, according to a time-honoured classification, are five in number—smell, sight, taste, hearing, and common sensation, or touch ; but such a classification of our sensations and of the organs which originate them is too crude for modern needs. Already we have shown that, whereas the nose and the tongue afford the same kind of information, the ear affords information of two, perhaps of three, different kinds. Within the realm of common sensation we pick out three special senses served by specialized sense-organs—touch, cold and heat—and, possibly, a fourth, served by non-specialized nerves, to which alone the epithet ‘“‘ common ”’ properly applies. The skin is supplied with nerves—naked fibrils—in the richest abundance. They are most easily demonstrated in the layer which covers the cornea, thanks to its transparency ; in this, as shown in Fig. 41, having branched on the front of the fibrous tissue of which the cornea is composed, the nerves pass towards the surface, forming connections with every one of its cells, or, at any rate, with every cell of the more superficial of the three or four layers of which the epithelium is made up. Ramified nerve-twigs of this type do not, under ordinary conditions, convey any sensations to consciousness. So long as the skin-cells with which they are connected are healthy, the nerve-twigs establish for them con- nections with the central nervous system by which their nutri- tion is regulated ; but they carry no impulses to which we can direct attention. The movement of blinking is accompanied by no sensation until the edges of the eyelids come in contact. A pencil pressed against. the lid evokes touch-sensations from the skin, but none from the cornea which underlies it. When 423 = ins se”) — 4 € Ok Se eee! U6 ee le “<> _. o pay ; *-. 4 — = =e r me SF a_i on Ss ‘ - ¥ ee te = 424 THE BODY AT WORK a tiny beetle injures the surface of the cornea by scratching — the epithelial cells with its horny wings and legs, the ruptured nerve-filaments convey to consciousness impulses, or, aS We — prefer to express it, an influence which is felt as pain. But even the pain caused by injury to the cornea is trifling as com- pared with that which originates in the under-sides of the lids, ' where not only is the epithelium supplied with branching nerve-twigs, but specialized organs of touch are present to localize the seat of injury. Everywhere the epithelium Fig. 41.—VERTICAL SECTION OF THE EPITHELIUM WHICH COVERS THE SURFACE OF THE CORNEA, AND OF A SMALL PORTION OF THE CORNEAL SUBSTANCE, HIGHLY MAGNIFIED. The black lines are naked nerve-fibres (stained with chloride of gold), which are distributed amongst the cells of the more superficial strata of the epithelium in very great abundance. The corneal substance is composed of sheets of transparent fibres with intervening cells. As the fibres of the several sheets cross one another at various angles, they are cut, some transversely, others in the direction of their length. covering the surface of the body is so abundantly supplied that a successful staining of nerve-filaments induces one to think that every epithelial cell has its nervous affiliation. These are the nerves of common sensation, if we retain the term ; but sensation so common, so obscure, so little differen- tiated that we know no more about it than we know about the air which envelops our hands and faces on a warm, windless day. Yet the air, when it moves, gives rise to a dim, broad, generalized sensation, which may be focussed into definiteness by a sensitive nerve. An observer who has devoted himself for many years to the investigation of skin-sensations, and especially of the “ re- ferred pains ’’ which are due to diseases of the viscera, recently caused the large cutaneous nerve which supplies the thumb a 2 ‘ ~ _SKIN-SENSATIONS 425 side of the forearm and hand to be cut in his own arm, in order that he might study carefully the revival of sensations. He found that he never lost his ability to recognize displace- ments of the tissues beneath the skin. Pacinian bodies and other end-organs of deep-lying nerves recorded pressure and tension caused by pushing or rubbing with a blunt instrument. Seven weeks after the injury he began to recognize stimuli that do harm—hot things, cold things, pricking with a pin— although his power of localizing the spot injured was extremely vague. In seven weeks, that is to say, the protopathic nerves, which do not follow the same definite lines as the nerves of the special senses, but form open networks with many alternative paths, had re-established their skin connections. Only gradu- ally and very slowly did critical sensations return—the ability to distinguish degrees of warmth, to recognize as separate two points of a pair of compasses, to feel a touch with cotton- wool. According to a theory set forth in this book (p. 312), pain is not a set of sensations, but a condition of the central nervous system which renders it unduly excitable, or excitable in a particular manner, to impulses which have the same local origin as the nerve-current which sets up the condition of pain. When a nerve of the skin has been cut, the epithelial ramifications are renewed before any specialized tactile or other sense-organs have regained their nervous connections. When the area which has regained its surface ramifications, but has not regained its sense-organs, is injured, no localization of pain results. Indeed, the obscure sensations which are then experienced if the skin be injured can hardly be described as painful. The ramified nerves pour their agitation into the grey matter of the spinal cord ; but it is not the agitation per se which causes pain. It is the passage of impulses through the agitated area that gives to them, when they reach conscious- ness, not only a topographical meaning, but also a distressful feeling. Until the specialized organs of the skin have been restored to working order, there are no impulses to pass through the agitated grey matter, and therefore no feelings of pain. According to this view there are two systems of afferent nerves, the protopathic and the specialized or critical. The former is very widely and very abundantly distributed to the surface 426 THE BODY AT WORK of the body, the lungs, the alimentary canal, and other viscera. — It has no end-organs, no defined tracts in the central nervous — system, no definite connections with the cortex of the great — brain. The currents which it conducts, if they originate in the — visceral part of this system, have no direct effect in conscious- ness ; but if they originate on the surface of the body, orinthe — alimentary canal at the lower end of the csophagus, or in certain other situations, they co-operate with stimuli of heat, cold, or traction. The critical system works in a more definite way. Its impulses originate in sense-organs. Starting with a certain potential, they are transmitted by the discharge of a succession of linked neurones. When they reach the cortex their potential is sufficiently high to evoke consciousness. Their distribution in the cortex is as definite as their origin. Specialized sense-organs are necessary for the origin of all sensations. Within the epithelium are certain cells which look as if they were specialized for sensory purposes. ‘The deeper sheet, or derma, of the skin is abundantly provided with struc- tures in which nerves end in the most elaborate and compli- cated ways (Fig. 42). They are found especially in the papille of connective tissue, which, set in rows, form the ridges that one can see at the finger-tips and in various other situa- tions. All of these organs are made up of groups of epithelial cells which, displaced from the epidermis, have sunk into the derma, with the nerves connected with them. In their further development the nervous part of the apparatus is complicated by branching, the branches being thickened and usually flattened into ribbons, which lie on the external surfaces of the cells or between them. A more or less marked capsule is provided for the organ by condensation of connective tissue. Anyone can convince himself that the skin is not uniformly sensitive. He may test it first for the minimal stimulus which excites a sensation of touch. With a hair of the head—it must not be a very fine one—cut across with scissors, and held between finger and thumb at the right distance from the cut end, the skin of the palm of the hand is prodded. Every here and there a spot is found which is insensitive to so slight a pressure. These spots are neither large nor very close together. If the hairless skin of the arm between the elbow and the 427 armpit be investigated in the same way, much larger blank areas are met with—oval patches more than } inch in diameter. When a hairy surface is tested, it is found that contact with a hair can always be felt ; and when the hairs are shaved, the touch-spots are found to extend around or from the points at which hairs pierce the epidermis. Touchless areas lie between them. Hair-follicles receive tufts of nerve-filaments, and it appears that they are the chief organs of touch. ‘‘ Touch- corpuscles,” which are found in great numbers in the papillz vi4 ats # O Z OF Nerve Fibre. Meduliary Sheath. £END-BULB. . TOUCH-CORPUSCLE. Fig. 42.—SENSE-ORGANS SUSCEPTIBLE TO PRESSURE. All are formed on essentially the same plan; a fibrous capsule invests a group of epithelial cells amongst which a nerve ramifies. The simplest form is known as a Grandry’s cor- puscle—a nerve ending in one or two plates between two or three epithelial cells. These organs are found in great numbers in the bills of aquatic birds. If a duckis watched whilst it is gobbling mud at the margin of a pond, it will be seen to have a remarkable capacity for discriminating between the shells of small snails, which it can crush, and stones, which it needs to drop from its bill. Its bill is also provided with small Pacinian corpuscles (Fig. 43). Touch-corpuscles, more elaborate in form than the one figured, are found in the papille of the skin of the fingers and elsewhere. They appear to be modified hair-follicles. End-bulbs occur in the conjunctiva and elsewhere, and especially in the peritoneum. Together with Pacinian corpuscles, they are accountable for sensations con- nected with the distension of the stomach and intestines. of the skin of the fingers and elsewhere, may probably be re- garded as, genetically, hair-follicles which have not developed hairs. If sensitiveness to pain is investigated by tapping very gently with a needle—or, better, by using a stiff horsehair fixed in a cleft stick, from which it projects about + inch—it will be found that every here and there are spots which are exceedingly sensitive, whilst adjoining them are areas which are moderately sensitive, and between these areas small spots or stretches of -: skin which do not give the anata oundlide even the horsehair be pushed untilitdoublesup. = Testing now for sensitiveness to cold with a cold plate point, “‘ cold-spots ” can be mapped on the skin. If the 1 } Ca. | ‘i, | ‘ ; A) ; y HWA (4 Perizeurium. Neurilemma --| |i ae Medullary Sheath.---g IEEE: Axon ~----- me Fic. 43.—PACINIAN CORPUSCLE. These organs are especially numerous in the neighbourhood of tendons and ligaments. They are also present beneath the skin of the hands and feet. Their capsules are formed of a great number of concentric lamelle of connective tissue, enclosing lymph-spaces. Within the capsule is a core of finely granular substance, which also shows a tendency to a lamellar disposition. The structure of these relatively large sense-organs is highly suggestive of sensitiveness to pressure, traction, or rubbing. is warmed to about 50° C., “ heat-spots”’ are found. The different kinds of spot are very irregularly distributed. They may coincide, or overlap, or leave blank spaces. Their relative abundance varies. In some regions touch-spots, in others cold- SKIN-SENSATIONS 429 : spots, in others heat-spots, are more closely grouped. The _ tongue and the hand, and especially the tips of the fingers, are most sensitive to touch; but whereas the tongue is also ex- ceedingly sensitive to warmth, the hands are relatively insensi- tive. Yet, speaking generally, parts especially sensitive to touch are little sensitive to temperature, and vice versa. Sensi- tiveness to cold is much more widespread than sensitiveness to heat. It is concentrated in the skin covering the abdominal viscera. A cold douche directed between the shoulders is doubtfully felt as cold. There is no doubt whatever about it when it strikes the skin over the stomach. From these observations it appears that the skin contains three sets of organs sensitive respectively to touch, cold, and heat. Certain investigators hold that it also contains specific organs, or nerve-endings, sensitive to painful stimulants ; but in this case there is the obvious difficulty of distinguishing between pain and touch. At no spot can pure pain be evoked free from any consciousness of touch. To a certain extent the combinations of epithelial cells and nerve-endings in the skin fulfil the negative requirement of sense-organs ; each kind, whilst specially sensitive to its own specific stimulant, is insensitive to stimulants of other kinds. But mutual exclusion is not absolute in the case of cold and warmth. If a warmed metal point be applied to a cold spot, it produces a sensation of cold. Our feelings of warmth and cold are to a large degree comparative. Luke- warm water feels cold to hands just taken out of hot water ; moderately cold water appears luke-warm to hands that have been in contact with ice. The sensory apparatus for cold and heat soon adapts itself, or, in physiological language, it is soon fatigued. If after a prolonged bath at the body temperature a foot be plunged into very hot water and withdrawn quickly, the feeling which first ensues is one of cold. It is indistinguish- able from the feeling provoked by dipping the foot into cold water. The sensation of cold subsequently gives place to one of painful warmth. This does not indicate that the heat-spots have been waked out of their lethargy by excessive stimula- tion. On the contrary, it is the cold-spots which, when they were first stimulated by the very hot water, answered “ Cold,” that now cry out “ Hot ”’; for both cold-spots and heat-spots, } take deta past, one Bg alr warm ad the othe applied simultaneously to two closely adjacent spo 3 the resulting sensation is “hot.” When the cold - withdrawn, or replaced by a second warm point, the ser ns sinks to “‘ warm.’ 7 CHAPTER XVI VOICE AND SPEECH A out carried horizontally backwards across the cartilage which projects forwards as Adam’s apple, a quarter of an inch below its notch, would show that it is V-shaped, the point of the Vin front. Each limb of the V isa broad plate. In the mid-line is a gap, the rima glottidis, through which the wind- pipe communicates with the pharynx (Fig. 45). It is overhung by the stiff leaf-shaped epiglottis, the edge of which can be felt with the finger behind the tongue. (yAwrris, the mouthpiece of a reed-pipe, is the term commonly used, for short, for the rima glottidis.) When air is being drawn into the lungs, the glottis is widely open. In speaking or singing it is almost closed. It is tightly shut whilst food is passing down the gullet. The glottis is bounded, as to its anterior two-thirds, by two membranous folds, the vocal cords. In its posterior third it has a triangular cartilage, the arytenoid, on either side. A distinc- tion is sometimes drawn between the anterior part, bounded by the vocal cords, and the whole glottis, the former being termed “ rima vocalis ”’; but it is scarcely justified, for, although it is true that the anterior part is essentially the organ of voice, and its margins alone vibrate when high notes are sung, the anterior ends of the arytenoid cartilages also vibrate during the production of low notes. (The substance of: these pro- cesses is not, properly speaking, cartilage ; it resembles the epiglottis in containing a great abundance of elastic fibres.) And here we must warn the reader not to picture to himself a vocal “cord ’”’ as a kind of fiddle-string. It bears no resem- blance to a cord, as we ordinarily understand the word ; it is but a fold of mucous membrane, such as one might pinch up between finger and thumb from the inner side of the cheek. 431 distended, and vast numbers of exceedingly slender | oh 3 oe fibres which traverse it. The first cartilage below the thyroid—it may be felt Be bic the finger—is termed “cricoid”’ (xpixos, a ring), from its. resemblance to a signet-ring. Narrow in front, its large signet = Here se seiner Epiglottis (Gy (3\-- Ayoid Bone ----/}\-- Ventricle _ Li. False Vocal Cord, -}-- Thyoarytenoid Muscle s --t--— Voeal Cord ---- Thyroid Cartilage , ~------- Cricoid Cartilage 0 O}------- Wind-pipe ()): (\ Fig. 44.—THE ANTERIOR HALF OF THE LARYNX SEEN FROM BEHIND. The drawing shows the folds of mucous membrane, the vocal cords, which stretch from the tips of the arytenoid cartilages to the recess behind the median portion of the thyroid cartilage. To the outer side of each vocal cord is seen the thyro-arytenoid muscle (cut across), consisting of a broad outer portion, chiefly concerned in closing the glottis during the act of swallowing, and a smaller internal portion, which regulates the length and the thickness of the segment of the cord allowed to vibrate. projects upwards, within the V of the thyroid, behind, and on the top of the signet rest the two arytenoids. Each arytenoid is a triangular pyramid, its anterior, external, and upper angles prolonged into processes. It is united with the cricoid by a swivel joint, which allows its anterior process to swing inwards or outwards under the influence of two antagonistic muscles attached to its outer angle—the lateral and posterior crico- arytenoids. Another muscle attached only to the arytenoids draws them together. Still another muscle—or two muscles, — =f , 3 7 f VOICE AND SPEECH 433 for it is in two separate bands—unites the anterior process of the arytenoid with the back surface of the thyroid just on the outer side of the attachment into that cartilage of the vocal cord. The internal thyro-arytenoid muscle is a comparatively narrow band; the external thyro-arytenoid muscle is thick and broad.* By the simultaneous contraction of the encircling wes B C D E Fig. 45.—THE APERTURE OF THE GLOTTIS SEEN FROM ABOVE. The leaf-like structure in front of it is the epiglottis ; the two triangular structures at the back, the arytenoid cartilages ; the white bands on either side, the vocal cords. A, The glottis is widely open during inspiration. Arrows show the lines of action of the muscles which rotate, and approximate, the cartilages. Attached to their outer angles, and pulling these _ angles forwards, the lateral crico-arytenoid muscles ; pulling them backwards and inwards, the posterior crico-arytenoid muscles. Drawing the cartilages together, the arytenoid muscles. 3B, The glottis during speaking in a deep chest-voice, or when a low note of the lower register is being sung. C, During the production of a high note of the lower register. D, During the production of a note of the head-register. E, During the act of swallowing ; the arytenoid cartilages are drawn towards the epiglottis ; the aperture is folded into a T ; the pharynx (the tube behind the glottis) is distended. muscles the larynx is closely squeezed together, the anterior portion of the slit forming a T, with the transverse limb in front. This occurs only in swallowing. Under the co-operat- ing contractions of the several muscles, the glottis assumes a variety of shapes. The external crico-arytenoids rotate the anterior angles of the arytenoid cartilages inwards (Fig. 45, A). * A bullock’s larynx is an admirable object of study. In almost all points of form and structure it is practically identical with the human larynx, and its large size makes it easy to dissect. 28 434 ‘ THE BODY AT WORK If at the same time the arytenoid muscle draws the cartilages together, the glottis is reduced to a slit (Fig. 45, C). The posterior crico-arytenoid muscles rotate the cartilages out- wards. If the arytenoid muscle is at the same time relaxed, the glottis gapes to its fullest extent (Fig. 45, A). The freer the opening, the less is the resistance to the blast of air, the gentler the vibrations of the cords, the lower the voice. The closer the slit, the greater is the resistance which the air in the windpipe has to overcome in passing through it, and consequently the more ample the vibrations into which it throws the vocal cords. The vocal cords are the tongues of a reed-pipe, which, com- mencing in the chest at the point where the great bronchi join to form the windpipe, comprises the larynx, and, above the larynx, the complicated chambers of the throat, mouth, and nasal cavities, including the spaces within the bones of the head which open out of them. The pitch of the voice depends upon (1) the length of the vocal cords, and (2) their tension. The first factor is fixed for every individual. The voice is base, © baritone, tenor, in a man ; contralto, mezzo-soprano, soprano, in a woman—in proportion as the cords are long, of medium length, or short. A man’s vocal cords measure, on the average, 15 millimetres, a woman’s 11 millimetres. When a boy is from twelve to fifteen years of age his vocal cords double in length, and the “ breaking ” of the voice occurs as he gives up trying to get high notes out of his longer cords, and allows them to produce manly tones of an octave lower. The lower posterior angles of the thyroid cartilages articulate with the cricoid. If the four cartilages are freed from all soft tissues without disturbing the thyro-cricoid, or crico-arytenoid joints, and if, while the thyroid is held in one hand, a finger of the other is placed on the front of the cricoid, it will be found that as this is depressed the arytenoid cartilages which rest upon its signet are tilted upwards and forwards within the - thyroid ; as it is raised, they are tilted away from it. In life this movement is effected by a muscle—the crico-thyroid - (Fig. 46)—attached to the front of the cricoid cartilage and to the under border of the lateral plate of the thyroid. This is the muscle of supreme importance in the production of the voice. The thyroid cartilage is slung in a fixed position by *» ee Pe © Pee Te oe a ae ie ety o Ot | ir pea hee ey if ae ee 2 al ie “ VOICE AND SPEECH 435 the hyoid bone (to be felt in the neck above it). The crico- thyroid muscle, being unable to depress the thyroid, raises the front of the cricoid cartilage, tilts back the arytenoids, tightens the vocal cords. As the voice ascends the scale, the tension of the cords is progressively increased, and their vibrations rendered proportionately more rapid. The range of the human voice is about three and a half octaves; of individual voices about two octaves ; if the shrill cry of a baby, which may reach the third G above the middle C, or even higher (E’’” or F’’”’), Fig. 46.—THE LARYNX FROM THE RIGHT SIDE. From above downwards: the hyoid bone, thyro-hyoid membrane, thyroid cartilage, cricoid cartilage, trachea. The upper and posterior angle of the wing of the thyroid cartilage is suspended from the hyoid bone ; its lower and posterior angle articulated with the cricoid cartilage. On the summit of the cricoid cartilage it articulates the arytenoid. Dotted lines indicate the position of the vocal cord. The crico-thyroid muscle, which raises the front of the cricoid, tilting the arytenoid cartilage backwards and tightening the vocal cord, extends, fan-like, from the front of the cricoid to the lower border of the wing of the thyroid. 7 be excluded. Exceptional voices have a range far greater than two octaves. Falsetto voice is produced by throwing half of the vocal cord out of vibration (the way in which this is accomplished is not clear), and at the same time raising the back of the tongue to the wall of the throat in such a manner as to cut off all the lower part of the upper resonating chamber, leaving it only the mouth and the cavities of the nose. So far the mechanism of voice is easily understood. As the scale is ascended, the vocal cords are progressively tightened by the contraction of the crico-thyroid muscles. But an analysis : 28—2 436 THE BODY AT WORK of the feelings experienced during singing (and of the quality of the sounds produced) shows that by themselves these muscles are not able to make changes in the tension of the cords sufficient to account for the full range of the voice. Or, put in another way, the tension of the vocal cords is not altered to the extent which would be necessary if upon it alone depended a range of from two to three octaves. It is obvious that by some means the length or thickness, or both, of the portions of the cords vibrating is changed as the scale is ascended. If commencement be made on a low note, a point is reached, after a certain number of notes have been sung, at which a sudden change occurs. There is an alteration in the quality of sound, the more marked, the less well trained the singer. The singer experiences a feeling of relief. If a finger be placed on his crico-thyroid muscle, a relaxation of its anterior fibres can be detected. As he proceeds up the scale, these fibres again tighten. At a certain point there is again a change in the quality of voice, and in the feelings which accompany its production. The two points at which change occurs are said to divide the voice into three “ registers ”—the lower, or chest-register, the middle, and the upper, or head-register. A great effort is needed to hold either register above its natural range. The physiology of the registers is a subject far too thorny for handling in this book. The larynx can be watched with the laryngoscope during the production of notes of different pitch, but observers are not in accord regarding the appearances which it presents, or their interpretation. The possibilities of changing the reed which vibrates, the vocal cord, otherwise than by increasing the direct pull upon it exerted by the crico- thyroid muscle, appear to be as follows : (1) During the pro- duction of the lowest notes the elastic portion of the arytenoid cartilage may be included with the cord. It may be thrown out of vibration by its rotation inwards (under the action of the lateral crico-arytenoid muscle) until it is pressed against its fellow. (2) Certain portions of the cord may be damped by partial contractions of the internal thyro-arytenoid muscle. It has been frequently stated, although the statement is not accepted by all anatomists, that some of the fibres which take origin from the arytenoid cartilage end in the cord, VOICE AND SPEECH 437 instead of passing right through to the thyroid. It is supposed that by their contraction they throw the posterior portion of the cord—even, it is asserted, as much as its posterior two- thirds in the higher head-notes—out of vibration. (3) It appears that the width (thickness) of the cord vibrating is also regulated by the contraction of the thyro-arytenoid muscle. Those who regard the diminution in the thickness and width of the vibrating fold of mucous membrane and underlying elastic tissue as the chief factor in the adaptation of the larynx for the middle register lay great stress upon the sense of relief from muscular effort which accompanies the transition. Less force is needed to tighten the thinner cord. They also call attention to the loss in volume of the voice when the lower register is left, and to its greater softness. The lower is spoken of as the thick register, the middle as thin, and the upper (on the hypothesis that part only of the cord eee as the small register. Singing reveals the possibilities of the Ltt as a musical instrument. In speech the larynx plays a part, but the form of the syllabic sounds and the relative prominence of over- tones in the vowels is of more importance than pitch. Flexi- bility of voice is dependent upon ability to increase or diminish at will the size of the resonating chambers of the throat. mouth, and nose, or the freedom of access to them. Con- versation is carried on in the lower or chest-register. When a practised speaker mounts a platform, he spends the first few minutes in ascertaining the pitch of the hall—that is to say, the pitch of his voice to which the room resonates most freely. Having found the proper tone, he endeavours to maintain a uniform tension of his vocal cords, and therefore a uniform pitch. He relieves the monotony of speech by suitable varia- tions of its overtones. Nothing is more uncomfortable to listen to than an oration delivered in cadences. The speaking voice should be full, round, and musical, and free from affecta- tion—as guiltless of the intoning or preaching quality as it is of harshness or of vulgar flatness. A flexible voice is capable of producing, as occasion calls for them, tones of any and every quality. With the throat and mouth set for the syllable “haw,” it is impossible to do justice to such words as “king ” and “queen.” The voice-tones of a superior person are’as 438 THE BODY AT WORK distasteful to the hearer as those of a vulgarian. Unpleasant also is a nasal twang, illogically so called, since it is due, not to the opening of the resonating chambers of the nose, but to the restriction of the entry of air into them. In this it is somewhat similar to the effect produced by a severe cold. Resonance in the nasal chambers produces a clear, ringing voice. A little consideration of the varying qualities of different voices suffices to show how largely they depend on resonance. When vowel-sounds are analysed, it is found that the dis- tinctive character of each of them is dependent upon the overtones which it contains. For every vowel the overtones are fixed, or very nearly so, no matter what may be the pitch of the note to which the vowel is sounded. It is much to be regretted that the alphabet was settled before the physiology of speech was understood. Were it based upon reasonable principles, children would be spared the bewilderment which overtakes them when they endeavour to establish in their minds some kind of relation between the names of consonants and their effects upon the blast of air as it passes through throat and mouth, and between tongue and palate, teeth and lips. The vowels, had physiologists defined them, would have been real pure vowel-tones—60, 0, ah, €—sounds which can be sustained for an indefinite time, and allowed to die away without deterioration in their quality. A (é as pronounced in France) is doubtfully pure—it has a tendency to tail off in @é; 4 is frankly a diphthong, az (ah-2). Try to hold a long final note on the syllable “nigh”! An international standard of vowel-sounds would have been fixed, by giving the vibrating periods of the tuning-forks for which in each several case the resonating chambers are shaped, and defining the relative accentuation of each overtone. Greatest boon of all, the irruption of the Essex dialect would have been dammed. It would not have been allowed to inundate London, or to submerge Australia, debasing our English tongue. In Cockney speech vowels degenerate down the line of greatest indolence. Aw becomes or, or ar; a becomes 2. It requires a greater effort to pronounce a full a than a flat a, a definite flat a than 7. And worse than a Cockney’s unwillingness to take the trouble necessary for the production of dignified VOICE AND SPEECH 439 vowel-tones is his reluctance to make the effort required for the holding of any tone. In his mouth virile, self-reliant vowels are replaced by emasculated diphthongs, which collapse as they present themselves to the ear. It costs trouble to fix the mouth-chamber before a vowel is sounded and to hold it steady until it is finished. Ah slides down through ai to 2; 7% slips into a. “Cow” becomes kyow; “ you,” ye-u-ow ; “cart,” kyart. And just as the effort needed for the filling of the vowels is shirked, so also is grudged the expenditure of an accessory blast for their aspiration. When a vowel is whispered, although the vocal cords do not vibrate, the blast passing through the resonating chambers produces the overtones characteristic of the vowel. Anyone who feels his own larynx while he sings, to the same note, the various vowels between 66 and @é—he may please himself as to the number of ai, eu, and % vowels he interposes between these two extremes—will recognize that it is pulled farther and farther upwards by the muscles which surround it. The cavity of the mouth is at the same time made shorter and broader for each succeeding vowel. Singing the several vowels before a piano, and at the same time striking various keys, it is felt in the mouth that the resonance of that chamber is reinforced by certain selected notes. Certain tuning-forks, when sounded in front of the mouth shaped for a vowel, ring out more loudly, because the mouth cavity resonates to their prime tones. The overtones of the vowels can be analysed in this way. Conversely, by sounding simultaneously an appro- priate selection of tuning-forks, each with the right degree of force, the overtones of a vowel can be synthesised. Thus if whilst one tuning-fork is sounding B,b (Bb above middle C), two others be added giving B,b (loud) and F, (soft), the com- posite sound resembles the vowel o. If to these same three forks, with F, sounding more strongly, B,b and a loud D, be added, the sound changes to ah. The organ of voice is a combination of a reed-pipe with resonating chambers, the shape of which can be changed at will. The quality characteristic of a vowel is given to it by adding to the note produced in the larynx sounds due to the resonance of the throat and mouth. On the assumption (not allowed by all authorities) that, since the resonating chambers ~ 440 THE BODY AT WORK are not sound-producers, they can only add to the Jarynx- — tone, as “formants ’’ of a vowel, its own harmonics—sounds which they have picked out of it—it follows that, if, when the prime is changed, the resonators were not adapted to the new note, they would be dumb. If this attitude in regard to the question be justified, there must be a certain amount of variation in the quality of a vowel as the scale is ascended. But a vowel is not a musical tone ; it is a conventional sound. Its whole value depends upon its retaining, as nearly as may be, the same quality, whatever be the pitch of its prime tone. By adjusting the form of the throat and mouth, we can not only prevent one vowel from passing into another, but we can keep it so nearly true to itself as to convince the ear that its quality is unchanged: 06 remains 60, and ah ah, although the form of the sound as produced on Cy is different to its form when sung to C. Apart from the general distinction that low notes are taken more easily with vowels requiring a large mouth-cavity, and high notes with those providing a small one, there are certain very distinct relations between vowel-sounds and musical tones which need to be borne in mind in setting words to music. A singer changes a word when he feels that its vowel-tone does not allow him to give to the note to which it is set the fullest expression of which he is capable. | An account of the physiology of the production of con- sonants is to be found in most text-books of grammar. INDEX AxssorpTIoN from alimentary canal, 129 Accelerator nerves of heart, 237 Accommodation of the eye for distance, 391 for light, 390 Acromegaly, 93 Addison’s disease, 91 Adrenalin, action on the kidney, 209 formed in suprarenal capsule, 92 Air, quantity inspired, 173 quantity needed by individual, 191 Air-cells of lungs, 168 Albumin made by plants, 12 Alcohol, effect on nerve conduction, 301 Alimentary canal, morphology of, 98 nerves of, 104 Altitude, highest, attained by climbers, 187 Alveoli of lungs, their number, 169 Amides produced from proteins, 119 Ameeba, irritability of its protoplasm, nitrite, effect on vascular system, 237 Anemia, treatment with iron, 67 Anesthetics, influence on protoplasm, 11 Analysis by animals, 12 Angina pectoris, 237 Angler fish, its nerve-cells, 31 Animal machine and its driver, 354, 358 Animals, hunting versus hunted, 366 not reflex machines, 358 relative insensibility to the knife, 361 Antitoxins, formation by protoplasm, Aorta, diameter of, 232 Aphasia, 352 Apnea, condition of arrested respira- tion, 181 Appendicitis, increased frequency of, Appetite, a safe guide, 114 Arteries, blood-pressure in, 234, 239 structure of wall of, 233 Artificial respiration, 183 Asphyxia, 182 Association zones in the cortex of the great brain, 348 Asthma, due to reflex contraction of small bronchi, 167 Astigmatism, correction by glasses, 393 due to modern print, 269 Attention, effect of, in heightening pain, 361 Bacteria, diminution of number in intestine on milk diet, 138 of alimentary canal, 135 of Bulgarian sour milk, 138 of the River Ganges, 141 in an infant’s intestine, 136 their réle in nature, 20 Balance-sheet of body, how drawn up, 149 Balloon, highest altitude attained in, 187 Basket-cells in nervous system, 324, 340 Bat’s squeak, number of vibrations, 418 Bats, flight not dependent on vision, 381 Beats in music, explanation of, 407 Beetle, muscle of, 261 Belladonna, physiological action, 109 Bile, composition, 117 function in regard to absorption of fat, 133 relation to digestion, 117 Bile-pigment, origin from hemoglobin, 69, 82, 118 Bioplasm, the essential substance of a living cell, 148 Birds, sense of hearing of, 410 Blind spot, how filled in, 395 Blisters, 41 Blood, amount ejected by heart, 219 circulation-time, 219 composition of, 59 gases of, amount, 190 tension, 61 lodged in abdominal veins, 234, 236 Blood-eorpuscles, cellular nature, 28 life-story, 62 number, 61 origin, 63, 64 structure, 60 Blood-platelets, 74 Blood-poisoning, 57 Blushing, 243 Bowman’s description of kidney, 200 discs in muscle, 259 Brain. Cf. Cerebellum, cerebrum blood-supply of, 352 Bread, digestion of, 120 Breathing, mechanism of, 171 Bruises, explanation of play of colours, 69 Bulgarian milk-germ, 138 Cortex of 44] PPAR Y OS sre Se Nol a ee en ee eT TP ey oe ee 442 Capillary vessels, circulation of blood migration of leucocytes from, ~ + 232 structure of their walls, 38 Carbohydrate foods, chemical composi- tion, 147 Carbonic acid, carried by blood, 60 liberation in lungs, 61, 189 Carbonic oxide, compound with hzemo- globin, 187 Carnivora, absorption of fat from alimentary canal of, 133 Cartilage, growth, 28 Catalysis, 17 Cell theory, 26 Cells, constituent parts, 26, 28 size, 30 specialization of function in, 35 Cells of Purkinje in the cerebellum, 303, 340 Cellulose, digestion of, 137 Cerebellum, cases of deficiency of, 341 - connections with cerebro - spinal axis, 340 development of granules of, 299, 303 lobes, 338 minute anatomy, 339 phylogeny, 338 relation to tone of muscles, 342 Cerebral hemisphere, an outgrowth to- wards olfactory pit, 334 in animals with various sen- sory endowments, 349 Cerebro-spinal fluid, 50 Chemical activity of protoplasm, 12 messengers, 89, 123 processes in plants, 15 Chemiotaxis of leucocytes, 56, 364 Children, brain in, 346 development of astigmatism in eyes of, 269 Chill, catching a, 242 Chloroform. C/. Anzsthetics Cholesterin, 118 Chromatolysis in nerve-cells, 320 Chrome-silver method of colouring nerve-tissue, 293 Chyme, food converted into, 126 Circulation of the blood, 218 Circulation-time, 219 Cirrhosis of liver, 42 Coagulation of blood, 69 Cochlea, anatomy, 413 Cockney dialect, the degradation of vowel-sounds, 439 Coke fire, poisonous fumes from, 186 Cold-spots in skin, 429 Collaterals of nerves, 297 Colon, length and disposition of, 101 Colour-blindness, 385 THE BODY AT WORK Colour vision, 385 Colours, reason for apparent fading in twilight, 378 Conductivity of protoplasm, 248 Consciousness, does not come within physiological investigation, 360 its part in animal life, 359 Control experiments, their value, 72 Convolutions of brain, 345 Cooking, effect upon digestibility of meat, 120 Corneal epithelium, sensitiveness of, 424 Corpus striatum of brain, 344 Cortex of cerebrum, discovery of ex- citability of, 344 - fissures and convolutions, 345 functional areas, 352 myelination of its fibres, 345 sensory and association areas, 346 structure of, 347 variations in different animals, 349 Corti, organ of, its structure, 414 theories of function of, 416 Coughing, mechanism of, 180 Crayfish, tone of claw-muscle of, 273 Cretinism, 85, 90 Cricket, chirp of, 261 Crypts of Lieberkiihn, 103 Curdling of milk, 75 Dancing, association of sound with movement, 422 Day’s work, food required for, 151 Deafness due to sore throat, 412 Degeneration of nerves after section, 326 Depressor nerve of the heart, 237 Diabetes, excretion of more carbo- hydrate than contained in food, 143 Dialysis, explanation of the process, 40, 128 Diaphragm, function in respiration, 171 Diastases, destructive ferments, 18 Diet, limits of possible variations in, 153 of labouring classes, 152 Digestibility of bread, meat, fish, etc., 120, 125 Digestion, mechanism of, 96 vascular changes during, 235 waits on appetite, 114 Digitalis, action on heart and kidney, 209 Diphtheria, antitoxin of, 20 Diuretics, 209 Dog’s sense of smell, 370 Dreams, theory of, 362 Dropsy, 42 oikga resuscitation from, 183 » Pp Dru ysiology of, 95 Ductless glands, 94 = Dyspnea, difficult respiration, 181 Ear, anatomy, 411 bones of, 412 differentiation into separate sense- organs, 410 in fishes, 410 phylogeny, 409 Eel’s blood injected into mammal, 20 Effector, an organ which exhibits change in response to stimulation, 253 Egg-albumin destroyed by blood, 19 Electric organs, 288 phenomena of muscles, 279 Emotions, their relation to vaso-motor changes, 242 Energy, expended by body, 151 source of the body’s, 152 of stimulus compared with energy of muscular response, 254 Engines, body compared with, 152, 256 Epiglottis during swallowing, 433 Equilibrium, maintenance of, in walk- ing, 342 Erepsin, ferment of intestinal juice, 119 Errors of sensory judgment, 402 Excretion, 195 Eye, accommodation for distance, 391 adaptation for darkness, 390 blind spot, 394 optical defects of, 393 phylogeny, 334 refractive media, image by, 391 Eyeball, abnormalities in shape of, 392 anatomy, 373 development, 374 muscles of, indefatigable, 269 formation of Fat, absorption of, 131, 132 accumulation of, relation to foods consumed, 144 chemistry, 132 digestion, 133 laid down in connective tissues, 145 stored in liver, 145 Fatigue, causes of, 45, 268 Fermentation, 16 Ferments, chemical nature, 18 . classification, 16, 18 physiological importance, 18 Fibrin of blood, its antecedents, 75 Fireflies, source of their light, 291 Fish, sense of smell of, 365 supposed to be frightened by noise, 410 Flatulence, cause of, 114, 125, 136 443 Foods, classification, 142 history of, after absorption. 142 relative value, 147, 151, 153, 157 residue after digestion and absorp- tion, 194 Foramen ovale of heart, sometimes per- forate, 218 Frigate-bird, turbinate bones of, 166 Frog, supposed to be found entombed in rock, 164 Functional interdependence of organs, 94 Functions transferred to other organs, 87 Gall-stones, cause of formation of, 118 Galvani’s observation of contraction of a frog’s muscles, 277 Ganges, purifying water of, 138 Ganglia of sympathetic chain, function, 325 Ganglion cells of retina, 376 spinal, 299, 333 Gaseous tension, meaning of expression, 188 Gases of blood, their exchange in the lungs, 184 Gastric glands, structure, 123 juice, amount secreted, 114 composition, 114 digestive action, 115 Gelatin as article of diet, 158 Giant cells, 65 Glands, vaso-motor nerves of, 109, 241 Glycogen, formula, 147 as muscle food, 148 stored in liver, 147 Goitre, cause of, 84 Granules, appearance of, in glands, 110 of cerebellum, development of, 299, 303 Grey matter, formation of paths in, 356 Growth, a function of protoplasm, 24 a reaction to work, 47 Hematin, 68 Heematoidin, 68 Hemochromogen, 68 Hemoglobin, crystalline form, 66, 186 formula, 66 as oxygen carrier, 66, 186 spectrum, 68, 185 Hemophilia, non-coagulability of blood, 76 Hallucinations, 362 Headache, a pain in the scalp, 106, 319 the brain’s warning of fatigue, 269 from strain of eye-muscles, 268 Hearing, analysis of compound vibra- tions, 405 capacity dependent upon educa- tion, 422 fe ee Se 44 + HE BODY ATWORK Hearing, Helmholtz’s theory of analysis of sounds, 419 range of sensations, 418 sense of, 404 upper limit, 418 Heart, anatomy, 217 automatism of, 238 development, 218 murmurs, 229 muscular tissue, minute structure, 261 nerves regulating beat, 237, 239 sounds of, 228 valves, their mechanism, 226 work done by, 219, 223 Heat, production of, by muscles, 254, 256 Heat-spots in skin, 429 Helmholtz’s theory of organ of Corti, 419 Hering’s theory of colour-vision, 388 Hormones, meaning of term, 89, 124 of pancreas and liver, 127 of stomach, 123 Humours in ancient medical theory, 79 Hunter, experiment of grafting cock’s spur in its comb, 47 Hydrochloric acid, part taken in diges- tion, 114 Hydrophobia, protective inoculation, 78 Hyperpnoea, excessive respiratory efforts, 182 Hypoblast, a layer of the embryo, 97 Illusions of movement, 335, 384 of size and distance, 400 Immunity, acquisition of, 20 Impulse of the heart, 225 rate of passage in muscle, 280 in nerve, 278, 280 theory of nerve conduction, 282 Inhibition, explanation of term, 311 of reflex actions, 311 ‘Insects, efficiency of their muscles, 261] Instinct, due to brain-pattern, 359 Intelligence of animals, 359 Internal secretions, 83 Intestinal juice, digestive action, 119 Intestine, large, sacculation of its walls, 101 small, folds and glands of mucous membrane, 102 Intestines, movements of, 103 nerves of, 105 size and situation, 100 Iodine, importance of, to economy, 89 Todothyrin, goitre due to deficiency of, 90 ‘Iris, its function in regulating admis- sion of light to eye, 394 Iron in food, 67 in hemoglobin, 67 use of, in treatment of anemia, 67 Irritability, a function of protoplasm, 10 Japanese, cultivation of sense of smell — y, 370 ‘ Judgment of angles, 402 of distance and size, 401 of meaning of sensations, 396, 399 Kidney, ancestral history, 195 vn elimination of indigo by, 207 — of birds and reptiles, 200, 207 hydrostatic mechanism, 189 minute anatomy, 196 Kinesthetic sensations, absence from dreams, 363 part played by, in voluntary actions, 354 representation in cortex o rain, 350, 352 Knee-jerk, 274 Labyrinth of ear, 413 Lactate of ammonia, relation to urea, 13 Lacteals, lymphatic vessels of ali- mentary canal, 43, 131 Lactic acid produced in muscle, 46, 146 Larynx, closure during swallowing, 433 structure of, 430 Latent period of muscle after nervous impulse reaches it, 278 Laughter, respiratory mechanism of, 180 Lecithin produced by metabolism of nerve-tissue, 118 Leech, ganglion cells of, 298 Leucocytes as protective agents, 52 death of, 54, 57 migration of, 49 number in lymph and in blood, 49, 61 origin of, 33, 51 source of fibrin-ferment, 74 Leucocythemia, excess of leucocytes in the blood, 215 Levers to which muscles are attached, 286 Light, emission of, by animals, 291 Lime, influence upon coagulation of blood, 75 curdling of milk, 75 Lithates, or urates, constituents of calculi, 213 Liver, destruction of red _blood-cor- puscles in, 83 form and structure of, 160 former theories of its functions, 129, 163 manufactures urea and uric acid, 146, 162 of well-fed sheep, 147 origin of, in vertebrate phylogeny, 34 Rate ee INTER Liver stores food, especiall lycogen, 46, 145, 147, 161 iain Locomotor ataxy, 341 — view of mechanism of kidney, Luminous glands, 291 Lung, exchange of gases in, 173, 184, 190 nerve-supply, 178 structure, 168 Lymph, amount of, in body, 37 composition, 49 relation to blood, 51 Lymph-spaces, 37, 43, 49 Lymphatic glands, structure of, 54 Lymphatic vessels, 43 Malapterurus, electric organs, 288 immense neurones of, 295 Manometer for measuring blood-pres- sure, description of, 238 Man’s ancestry, 153 Massage of abdominal viscera, 101 of muscles, 48 Meal, the story of a, 120 Meat, diet consisting solely of, 157 digestion of, 121 extracts of, as articles of diet, 159 Megacaryocytes, 65 Memory, physiological explanation, 356 Metabolism, chemical change in living tissue, 12, 273 Methzmoglobin, 69 Microscope, its discovery, 26 Migration of birds, 359 Milk, call for secretion of, by a hormone, chemical and physical constitu- tion, 132 digestion of, 127 Milk diet, reduction of bacteria in ali- mentary canal on, 138 Mind, physiology of, 354 Mosquitoes, production of sound by, 261 Motile cells, 32 Mountain sickness, 187 Mountains, highest climbed, 187 Mucous membrane, use of term, 97 Murmurs, in chest, in diseases of lungs, 169 of heart, 229 Muscle, change in appearance under microscope during contraction, 263 chemistry of contraction, 266 coniraction a phenomenon of os- mosis, 258 electric phenomena of, 278 means of promoting growth of, 271 measurement of its power, 285 nature of impulse which leads to contraction of, 282 445 ~ Muscle of heart, its minute structure, 224 of insects, its efficiency, 261 plain, its minute structure, 258 plasma, its coagulation, 266 rhythm of voluntary contraction, 279 : theory of its structure as a mechan- ism liberating energy, 234, 255 tone of, 272 tracings taken of contracting, 278 voluntary, its minute structure, 259 wastes when its nerve is severed, 274 work done by, proportional to load, 286 Muscles, arrangement in regard to the bones which they move, 286 co-operation in lifting a weight, 287 Muscular energy, source of, 235 Muscularis mucose of alimentary canal, 103 Musculi papillares of heart, 227 Music, chords admissible in, 408 Indian, division of octave, 408 primitive, prevalence of minor chords, 408 Musical tones and overtones, 406 Myelination of nerves, order of, 345 Myxcedema, dependent on disease of thyroid gland, 85 Myxomycetes, fusion of cell bodies of, 27 Nasal chambers, air warmed in, 166 Negroes, their long heels, 285 Nerve, conduction in, theory of, 282 degeneration, 326 electrical phenomena, 279 indefatigable, 282 regeneration, 326 structure, 296 Nerves, depressor, 237 experiment of crossing, 327 fifth, 316 of heart, 239 of intestines, 426 protopathic and critical systems of, 425 secretory, of the salivary glands, 109 splanchnic, 236 superior laryngeal, 178 vagus, 104 vaso-motor, 239 Nerve-cells last throughout life, 148 limitations of their functions, 321 store of energy in, 320 transfer of impulses from cell to cell, 177, 300 their relation to muscle-fibres, 274 ' varying size of, 295, 322 Nerve-centres, 176 cer improper use of expression, 81 Nerve-impulses, distribution in grey -matter, 305 reinforcement of, 320 resistance to, at synapses, 306 Nerve-nets, pericellular, 301, 319 Nervous system, neuronic and extra- neuronic conduction, 310 phylogeny of, 332 Neuro-fibrille, 298 Neurone, origin of term, 293 transmission of current by, 328 various types of, 296, 323 N ight- blindness, 378 Nissl’s bodies, source of nervous energy, 320 Nitric oxide, combination with hzemo- globin, 186 Nitrogenous equilibrium, 150 food, stimulating effect of, 157 waste, 210 Neud vital of Flourens, 176 Normal diet, 151 Normal salt solution, 82 Nucleo-proteins, source of uric acid, 215 Odours, classification of, 366 (Edema, or dropsy, 42 Olfactory membrane, structure, 366 Optic nerve, number of fibres, 378 Organ of Corti, structure, 415 theory of function, 417 Organs that have lost their prime func- tions, 87 Orientation, sense of, 335 Osmosis, 40, 128, 201 cause of muscular contraction, 235 Osteoblasts, bone-forming cells, 32 Osteoclasts, bone-eating cells, 65 Oxygen, amount required per diem, 166 carried by red blood-corpuscles, 66 Pain, influence of, upon action, 359 referred from viscera to surface of body, 316 relation to sensation, 313, 425 theory of, 312, 425 Pancreas, structure, 116 Pancreatic juice, constitution, 116 fat-splitting ferment of, 133 Papille of the tongue, various forms of, Parathyroids, 86 Pepsin, digestive action, 115 Peptone prevents coagulation of blood, 717 Pericellular nerve-nets, 301 Perspiration, cools the surface of the body, 236 repressed during fever, 257 Poyer’ s SERS: of lymph follicles in is ee 53 : ony, 6. agocytes, germ-eating leuc yi fan of red blood - cor- puscles by, 82 Phosphenes, developed by pressure on eyeball, 383 Phosphorescence, cause of, 291 Phrenology; 343 Pictures, suggestion of solidity in, 401 — Pineal body, phylogeny, 334 Pituitary body, Plants, Edler ffi’ by ether, 12, 24 their metabolism, 15 their respiration, 24 Pleura, lining membrane of chest, 172 Pleurisy, pain of, 313 Pleuritic fluid, absorption of, 223 Pneumonia, changes in lung during, 169 Portal system of bloodvessels, 80 regulator of vascular tone, 236 Power of muscles, 285 Precipitins formed in blood, 19 Proteins, absorption by alimentary canal, 145 chemical constitution, 6 dietetic value, 157 fate after absorption, 212 Protopathic nerves, 425 Protoplasm, arrangement in cells, 30 constitution, 7 Huxley’s ae 6 Pulse, cause of, 2 records of, 248 variations, 247 Purgatives, theory of action, 128 Purkinje cells of cerebellum, 303, 340 shadows of retinal vessels, 375 Pus, origin of, from leucocytes, 57 Pyramids of cortex of great brain, 346 Rabbit’s ear, vaso-motor changes in, 235 Receptor, an organ specially sensitive to stimulation, 253 Referred pains from viscera, 316 Reflex action, inhibition of, 311 of scratching, 330 vinegar experiment with frog, 307 Regeneration of nerves, 326 Renal-portal circulation, 199 Renewal of tissues, 148 Rennin, ferment of milk, 16 Resistance in nervous system, laws of, 177, 307 Respiration, artificial, 179 effect on circulation, 221 a function of protoplasm, 23, 164 movements of, 171 nervous mechanism, 175, 179 in tissues, 165, 193 . Respiratory centre in medulla oblon- gata, 17 > 178, 182 Respiratory quotient, 174 Retina, structure, 374 Retinal pigment, relation to vision, 381 Rice ordeal, arrest of secretion of saliva, 112 Rigor mortis, 266 Rods and cones, respective functions in vision, 378 Rowing, value of, as exercise, 287 Saccharin, taste of, 367 Saline frog, respiration in, 193 Saliva, chemical constitution, 107 function of, 96, 107 Salivary glands, mechanism of secre- tion, 108 nerves of, 109, 236 Salts, absorption of, in alimentary canal, 128 Scientific method, definition of, 71 Scratch reflex, in dog, 330 Sea-sickness, 106 Secretin, hormone of pancreas and liver, 127 Secretion, accumulation of granules in cells, and their discharge, 110 a response to stimulation, 111 not a process of filtration, 110 Semicircular canals, their functions, 410 their positions in space, 335 Sensations, their apparent fusion, 356 many which escape attention, 318, 355 neutralization of one by another, 356 Sense-organs, origin in vertebrata, 336 Sensory areas in cortex of the great brain, 348 Sensory nerves, their connection with cerebro-spinal axis, 304 Shell-fish, poisonous extract of, 41 Shivering due to loss of heat from skin, 257 Sight. Cf. Vision Skate, electric organs of, 289 Skilled movements, dependent upon kinesthetic sensations, 357 Skin, experiment of cutting nerve, 424 variety of sensations from, 423 Sleep, condition of neurones in, 362 Sleeping sickness, 33 Smallpox, protection against, 78 Smell, disappearance of sense of, in later life, 370 dog’s dependence upon sense of, 366 reason for mental associations with sensations of, 371 sensitiveness to mercaptan, 365 Smells, nice and nasty, 369 447 Smoking, mental effect of, 371 Sneezing on looking at bright light, 317 Sore-throat, cause of deafness, 412 Soul, Aristotle’s definition, 32 Sound, mode of conduction, 404 rapidity of vibrations of, 406, 418 Sounds of the heart, 228 periodic and aperiodic, 409 Spectacles, defects of eyeball which call for, 392 Speech, derangements of, due to dissase of the brain, 353 mechanism of, 437 Sphygmographs for recording pulse, 245 Spinal dog, reflex action in, 330 frog, reflex action in, 307 ganglia, development of cells, 299 Splanchnic nerves, regulation of blood- pressure by, 236 Spleen, destruction of blood-corpuscles in, 80 structure, 79 Squint, correction of double vision in, 397 Starch, formula, 15 Star-shapes due to puckering of crys- talline lens, 393 Starvation, statistics of, 156 Stiffness of muscles, cause of, 45, 271, Stimuli to muscles and nerves, 248 Stokes, discovery of spectrum of blood, 68 Stomach, digestion in, 120 glands of, 123 referred pains from, 316 shape and size, 99 Stone in the bladder, its cause, 213 Subconscious self, 355 Sugars, digestion of, 120, 136 formule, 15 Sun, apparent size near horizon, 399 Suprarenal capsules, their structure and function, 91 Sweetbread as article of diet, 215 Sympathetic system of nerves, 243, 325 diameter of fibres, 325 Synapses of nerve-cells, resistance inter- posed at, 306 Synaptases, constructive ferments, 18 Synthesis by plants, 15 Tapeworms, resist digestion in the in- testines, 21 Taste, confusion with sense of smell, 364 localization on tongue, 367 sense of, in fishes, 365 sensitiveness to quinine, 369 Taste-bulbs, their structure, 368 Tattooing, removal of pigment by leuco- cytes, 55 Tea, its dietetic value, 122 ye Beate Oi ee a oy « in a a) , . 448 - Teeth, 96 Tendon, the growth of, from cells, 28 Tension of gases in the lungs, 190 Tetanus, the vibratile contraction of mustle, 279 Thoracic duct, discharges lymph into veins, 43, 131 Thorax, negative pressure in, 222 Thorns on Thyroid body or gland, forms an in- ternal secretion, 86 relation to goitre, 85 structure of, 85 Tight-lacing, deformation of organs which it causes, 220 Tigroids, in nerve-cells, stores of energy, 320 Tissues, respiration in, 165, 193 Tone of muscles, 272 Tongue, as organ of taste, 367 Tonsils, function as guardians of the fauces, 53 structure, 52 Torpedo, electric organs of, 290 Touch, sensations of, 426 Toxins produced by microbes, 20 Urea, amount relatively to proteins consumed, 155 antecedents of, 146, 212 chemical formula, 211 secreted during period of starva- tion, 156 Uric acid, amount secreted daily, 213 artificial production of, 13 chemical formula, 13, 214 diathesis, its relation to diet, 140 due to metabolism of leuco- cytes, 53, 216 form in which excreted, 207 made in the liver of birds, 13 Urticaria due to abnormal composition of lymph, 41 Vaccination, protective value of, 22 Valves of heart, their mechanism, 226 THE THE BODY AT ‘WORK endrites of nerve-cells, 300 - Vascular svat tone of, 236, 240 Vaso-constrictor nerves, 236 Vaso-dilator nerves, 236. Vegetables, dietetic value of, 139 digestion of, 125, 137 Vermiform appendix, 88 Villi of intestine, absorption of food by, 130 fat seen in, during active di- gestion, 134 Viscera, their insensitiveness to injury. 316, 426 Vision, colour contrasts, 382 duration of images, 382 judgment of distance and size, 411 solidity, 401 stereoscopic, doctrine of corre- sponding points, 397 Visual purple, 381 Vital action, definition of expression, 205 Vivisection, 4 Vocal cords, structure, 431 how modified in singing, 435 Voice, breaking of, in boys, 434 falsetto, how produced, 435 range of human, 435 registers, 436 Vomiting, 105 Vowels, synthesis by tuning-forks, 439 Wandering cells, 33 Warmth, appreciation of, by skin, 429 Waste substances, classification, 194 how eliminated from body, 59 Waterfall, negative after-image of, 384 Water-weed, experiment proving that it respires, 24 Wear and tear of bioplasm, 145 Wisdom-tooth, tending to disappear, 96 Yawning, beneficial effect on circula- tion, 222 nervous mechanism of, 180 Young’s theory of colour-vision, 385 Zymogen, 110 END BILLING AND SONS, LTD., PRINTERS, GUILDFORD Somes re ae er Beater ey” sw My Dr. Hutchison’s Works. FOOD AND THE PRINCIPLES OF DIETETICS. By ROBERT HUTCHISON, M.D. Epin., F.R.C.P., © Assistant Physician to the London Hospital and t» the Hospital for Sick Children, Great Ormond Street. xx+582 pages, with 3 Plates in Colour and 34 Illustrations in the text. Demy 8vo., red buckram, 16s. net. NEW AND REVISED EDITION. OUTLINE OF CONTENTS. CHAPTER : I. The Nature, Nutritive Constituents, and Relative Values of Foods, II. The Amount of Food required in Health. III. On the Influence of Various Conditions upon the Amount of Food re- quired. IV. Animal Foods. V. Jellies—Fish. VI. Soups, Beef-Extracts, Beef-Juices, Beef-Tea, and Beef-Powders. VII. Milk. VIII. Foods derived from Milk. IX. Oheese, Eggs, and Egg Substitutes. X. Vegetable Foods. XI. The Cereals: Wheat—Bread. XII. Bread (continwed)—Other Cereals. XIII. The Pulses—Roots, and Tubers. XIV. Vegetables—Fruits—Nuts—Fungi—Alge and Lichens XV. Sugar, Spices, and Condiments, XVI. Mineral Constituents of the Food. XVII. Water and Mineral Waters. XVIII. Tea, Coffee, and Cocoa, XIX. Alcohol. XX. Alcoholic Beverages: Spirits and Malt Liquors. XXI. Alcoholic Beverages (continued): Wines. XXII. The Cooking of Foods. XXIII. The Digestion of Food in Health. XXIV. The Principles of Feeding in Infancy and Childhood : Human Milk. XXV. The Principles of Feeding in Infancy and Childhood (continued): Sub- stitutes for Human Milk. XXVI. The Principles of Feeding in Infancy and Childhood (continued) : Other Substitutes for Human Milk (Peptonized Milk, Condensed Milk, Pro- prietary Foods) ; Feeding of Older Children. XXVII. The Principles of Feeding in Disease. XXVIII. The Principles of Feeding in Disease (continued). XXIX. Artificial and Predigested Foods and Artificial Feeding, LONDON : EDWARD ARNOLD, 41 & 48 MADDOX STREET, W. Dr. Hutchison’s Works. LECTURES ON DISEASES OF | CHILDREN. By ROBERT HUTCHISON, M.D. Epin., F.R.C.P., Physician to the London Hospital and Assistant Physician to the Hospital for Sick Children, Great Ormond Street. Author of ‘* Food and the Principles of Dietetics.” THIRD IMPRESSION. xii +338 pages. With Illustrations. Crown 8vo., cloth, 8s. 6d. net (post free 8s. 10d.). CONTENTS. CHAPTE R I. The Clinical Examination of Sick Children. II. The Artificial Feeding of Infants. III. The Digestive Disorders of Infancy—Colic and Vomiting. IV. The Digestive Disorders of Infancy (continwed)—Diarrhea. V. The Wasting Diseases of Infancy—Marasmus and Congenital Syphilis. VI. Tuberculosis in Childhood. VII. Rickets. VIII. Infantile Scurvy. IX. The Dyspepsias of the Second Dentition. X. Rheumatism in Childhood. XI. The Respiratory Diseases of Children. XII. Some Functional Nervous Diseases of Childhood. XIII. Some Functional Nervous Diseases of Childhood (continued). XIV. The Paralyses of Childhood. XV. Meningitis. XVI. On Mental Deficiency in Childhood. XVII. The Blood Disorders of Early Life. XVIII. Some Common Symptoms of Disease in Children and their Diagnostic Significance. XIX. Some Medical Aspects of Adenoid Vegetations in Infancy and Childhood. APPLIED PHYSIOLOGY. A Handbook for Students of Medicine. By ROBERT HUTCHISON, M.D., F.R.C.P. Crown 8vo., 7s. 6d. net (post free 7s. 10d.). The author of a standard work on diet is not likely to err by being too theoretical. The principle of Dr. Hutchison’s new book is to bring physiology from the laboratory to the bedside. ‘‘ Physio- logy,” he writes, “is studied in the laboratory, and clinical medicine in the wards, and too often one finds that the student is incapable of applying his scientific knowledge to his clinical work.” LONDON : EDWARD ARNOLD, 41 & 48 MADDOX STREET, W. 2 ¢ Mr. Edward Arnold’s Books on Biology. THE EVOLUTION THEORY. By Dr. AUGUST WEISMANN, Professor of Zoology in the University of Freiburg in Breisgau. Translated, with the Author’s co-operation, By J. ARTHUR THOMSON, Regius Professor of Natural History in the University of Aberdeen, AND MARGARET THOMSON. Two vols., xvi+416 and viii+396 pages, with over 130 Illustrations. Royal 8vo., cloth, 32s. net (post free 32s. 7d.). ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR. By Prorgssor C. LLOYD MORGAN, LL.D., F.R.S., Principal of University College, Bristol. viii +344 pages, with 26 Illustrations. Crown 8vo., cloth, 7s. 6d. net | (post free 7s. 11d.). HABIT AND INSTINCT. By Prorressor C. LLOYD MORGAN. viii + 352 pages. Demy 8vo., cloth, 16s. A TEXT-BOOK OF ZOOLOGY. By G. P. MUDGE, A.R.C.Sc. Lonp., F.Z.S., Lecturer on Botany and Zoology at the London School of Medicine for Women (University of London) and at the Regent Street Polytechnic; and Demonstrator on Biology at the London Hospital Medical College (University of London). xvi+416 pages, with 2 Coloured Plates and numerous original Illustrations. Crown 8vo., cloth, 7s, 6d. THE LIFE OF THE SALMON. With Reference more especially to the Fish in Scotland. By W. L. CALDERWOOD, F.R.S.E., Inspector of Salmon Fisheries for Scotland. Illustrated. Demy 8vo., 7s. 6d. net (post free 7s, 10d.). A CLASS-BOOK OF BOTANY. By G. P. MUDGE, A.R.C.Sc. Lonp., F.Z.S., AND ARTHUR J. MASLEN, F.L.S., Lecturer on Botany at the Woolwich Polytechnic, xvi+512 pages, with over 200 Illustrations. Crown 8vo., cloth, 7s. 6d. HOUSE, GARDEN, AND FIELD. A Collection of Short Nature Studies, By L. C. MIALL, F.R.S., Professor of Biology in the University of Leeds, and Fullerian Professor of Physiology in the Royal Institution. With numerous Illustrations. viii+316 pages. Crown 8vo., cloth, 6s. LONDON: EDWARD ARNOLD, 41 & 48 MADDOX STREET, W. 3 A TEXT-BOOK OF EXPERIMENTAL PSYCHOLOGY. By C. 8. MYERS, Professor of Psychology at King’s College, London University. Crown 8vo., 7s. 6d. net (post free 7s. 11d.). - For some time past the lack of a text-book on Experimental Psychology has been keenly felt. The literature of the subject is now so scattered and so profuse that a student must have at his command a small library of books and periodicals if he wishes to pursue a course of independentreading. Accordingly, it is the object of the forthcoming work to give an account of the more important results that have been obtained in this field of research, as well as to describe the methods and principles of psychological experiment. To each chapter an account is appended of experiments which may be performed by the rious in the laboratory, and illustrations are given of the apparatus employed therein. AN INTRODUCTION TO CHILD-STUDY. By W. B. DRUMMOND, M.B., O.M., F.R.C.P.E., Medical Officer and Lecturer on Hygiene to the Edinburgh Provincial Committee for the Training of Teachers. Author of ‘‘The Child: His Nature and Nurture.” Crown 8vo., cloth, 6s. net (post free 6s. 4d.). CONTENTS. Preparation—Caution in Child-Study—Biology and Child-Study—The Methods of Child-Study—How to Study a Baby—Weights and Measures, some Facts of Growth—The Senses and the Nervous System—The Health of the Child— Fatigue--The Instincts of Children—Instinct and Habit—The Interests of Children—Forms of Expression—Some Moral Characteristics—Religion and the Child—Peculiar and Exceptional Children. THE CHILD’S MIND: ITS GROWTH AND TRAINING. By W. E. URWICK, M.A. Crown 8vo., cloth, 4s. 6d. net (post free 4s. 10d.). CONTENTS. The Bearing of Biology on Educational Theology—The Mechanism of Learning— Mind’s Main Characteristics and Early Products—Lower Processes of Learning —Learning and the Growth of Ideas—Final Values of Learning—Growth of Ideas in Imaginative Process: Normal Types of Learning—Reasoning as Mind’s Emergency Process—Growth of Immediate Values—Some Conclusions. PSYCHOLOGY FOR ‘TEACHERS. By ©. LLOYD MORGAN, LL.D., F.R.S., Principal of University College, Bristol. New Edition, entirely rewritten. xii+307 pages. Crown 8vo., cloth, 4s. 6d. For this edition, Professor Lloyd Morgan has entirely rewritten, and very consider- ably enlarged, his well-known work on this important subject. He has, in fact, practically made a new book of it. LONDON: EDWARD ARNOLD, 41 & 48 MADDOX STREET, W. 4 a Mr. Pawar Arnold’s List of | Technical & Scientific Publications Extract from the LIVERPOOL POST of Dec. 4, 1907:— “During recent years Mr. Edward Arnold has placed in the hands of engineers and others interested in applied science a large number of volumes which, independently altogether of their intrinsic merits as scientific works, are very fine examples of the printers’ and engravers’ art, and from their appearance alone would be an ornament to any scien tific student’s library. Fortunately for the purchaser, the publisher has shown a wise discrimination in the technical books he has added to his list, with the result that the contents of the volumes are almost without exception as worthy of perusal and study as their appearance is attractive.” : The Dressing of Minerals. By HENRY LOUIS, M.A, Professor of Mining and Lecturer on Surveying, Armstrong College, Newcastle-on-Tyne. With about 4oo Illustrations. Royal 8vo., 30s. net. The object of this book is to fill a gap in technological literature which exists between works on Mining and works on Metallurgy. On the interme- diate processes, by which the minerals unearthed by the miner are prepared for the smelter and for their use in arts and manufactures, no English text-book has yet appeared. The present work should, therefore, be very welcome to students, as well as to miners and metallurgists, ARNOLD’S GEOLOGICAL SERIES. GENERAL EDITOR: DR. J. E. MARR, F.R.S. The economic aspect of geology is yearly receiving more attention in our great educational centres, and the books of this series are designed in the first place for students of economic geology. It is believed, however, that they will be found useful to the-student of general geology, and also to surveyors and others who are concerned wiih the practical appli- cations of the science. The Geology of Coal and Coal-Mining. By WALCOT GIBSON, D.Sc., F.G.S. 352 pages. With 45 Illustrations. Crown 8vo., 7s. 6d. net (post free 7s. rod.). Many years’ professional experience among the coalfields of this country and of South Africa have enabled the author to treat his subject in a thoroughly original manner. The book therefore contains a great amount of valuable information, as well as many criticisms and suggestions which have not hitherto appeared in any text-book on the subject. IN PREPARATION. The Geology of Ore Deposits. By H. H. THOMAS anp D. A. MACALISTER, Of the Geological Survey. Illustrated. Crown 8vo., 7s, 6d, net (post free 7s, 1od.). LONDON: EDWARD ARNOLD, 41 & 43 MADDOX STREET, W. bah “Electrical Trackow } ih By ERNEST WILSON, Wut. Scu., M.I. E. ee a Professor of Electrical Engineering in the Sieh ena Laboratory, King’s College, London, — AND FRANCIS LYDALL, B.A., B.Sc. NEW EDITION. REWRITTEN AND GREATLY ENLARGED. Two volumes, sold separately. Demy 8vo., cloth. Vol. 1., with about 270 Illustrations and Index. Vol. II., with about 170 Illustrations and Index. I5s. net each volume (post free 15s. 5d.). ‘‘ We are most decidedly of the opinion that both of these volumes will prove of great value to engineers, and that the last volume, in view of the present great interest in the question of single-phase traction, is of the utmost importance, for in it for the first time is published a great amount of data with reference to which, hitherto, the manufacturing companies concerned have observed great secrecy.”"—7" he Times (Engineering Supplement). A Text-Book of Electrical Engineering. By Dr. ADOLF THOMALEN. Translated by G. W. O. HOWE, M.Sc., Wut. Scu., A.M.LE.E., Lecturer in Electrical Engineering at the Central Technical College, South Kensington. With 454 Illustrations. Royal 8vo., cloth, 15s. net (post free 15s. 6d.). This translation of the “ Kurze Lehrbuch der Electrotechnik ” is intended to fill the gap which appears to exist between the elementary text-books and the specialized works on various branches of electrical engineering. Alternating Currents. A Text-Book for Students of Engineering. By C. G. LAMB, M.A., B.Sc., Clare College, Cambridge ; Associate Member a the Institution of Electrical Engineers ; : Associate of the City and Guilds of London Institute. vili+ 325 pages. With upwards of 230 Illustrations. Demy 8vo., cloth, Ios. 6d. net (post free 10, I1d.). Electric and Magnetic Circuits. By ELLIS H. CRAPPER, M.1I.E.E., meread of the Electrical Engineering Department in the University College, Sheffield. viii+380 pages. Demy 8vo., cloth, Ios. 6d. net (post free 1os. 11d.). Applied Electricity. A Text-Book of Electrical Engineering for ‘‘ Second Year” Students. By J. PALEY YORKE, Head of the Physics and Electrical Engineering Department at the London County Council chool of Engineering and Navigation, Poplar. xii+ 420 pages. Crown 8vo., cloth, 7s. 6d. MM — : i bs 7 4 nie ssw he F : ~ =~ i “i of le aa ul. 4 cs. : _ By’F. C. LEA, B.Sc., A.M.INST.C.E., vorth Scholar, A.R.C.S.; Lecturer in Applied Mechanics and Engineering Design, _ City and Guilds of London Central Technical College, London. ‘ , With 367 Diagrams. Demy 8vo., 18s. net (post free 18s. 5d.). _ This book is intendedyto supply the want felt by students and teachers q alike for a text-book of Hydraulics to practically cover the syllabuses of London and other Universities, and of the Institution of Civil Engineers. Hydraulics. By RAYMOND BUSQUET, Professeur & I’Ecole Industrielle de Lyon.- AUTHORIZED ENGLISH EDITION. Translated by A. H. PEAKE, M.A., Demonstrator in Mechanism and Applied Mechanics in the University of Cambridge. vilit+ 312 pages. With 49 Illustrations. Demy 8vo., cloth, 7s. 6d. net ; (post free 7s, 10d.). sz The Balancing of Engines. By W. E. DALBY, M.A., B.Sc., M.INST.C.E., M.I.M.E., Professor of Engineering, City and Guilds of London Central Technical College. SECOND EDITION, REVISED AND ENLARGED. xli+ 283 pages. With upwards of 180 Illustrations. Demy 8vo., cloth, Ios. 6d. net (post free ros. Iod.). Valves and Valve Gear Mechanisms. BY W. . DALBY, M.A.;. B.Sc. M.INST.C.E....M.1M.E:. Professor of Engineering, City and Guilds of London Central Technical College. xvili+ 366 pages. With upwards of 200 Illustrations. Royal 8vo., cloth, 21s. net (post free £1 Is. 5d.). Power Gas Producers. Their Design and Application. By PHILIP W. ROBSON, Of the National Gas Engine Co., Ltd. ; sometime Vice-Principal of the Municipal Schoul of Technology, Manchester. Demy 8vo., cloth, 10s. 6d. net (post free 10s. 11d.). The recent enormous increase in the use of gas power is largely due to the improvements in gas producers. This book, which is written by a well-known expert, goes thoroughly into the theory, design, general management. and application of all kinds of plants, with chapters on working and» |The Mudcat hd { miasdeny: of Stru . 3 L z Members. | oe By R. J. WOODS, M.E., M.INsT.C.E., _ Formerly Fellow and Assistant Professor of Roginseciag, Royal Indian Engineering College, Cooper’s Hill. SECOND EDITION, REVISED. xii+310 pages. With 292 Illustrations. Demy 8vo., cloth, tos. 6d. net (post free ros. tod.). Calculus for Engineers. By JOHN PERRY, M.E., D.Sc., F.R.S., Professor of Mechanics and Mathematics in the Royal College of Science, London ; Vice-President of the Physical Society; Vice-President of the Institution of Electrical Engineers. NINTH IMPRESSION. viiit+382 pages. With 106 Illustrations. Crown 8vo., cloth, 7s. 6d. Mathematical Drawing. Including the Graphic Solution of Equations. By G. M. MINCHIN, M.A,, F.RS., Formerly Professor of Applied Mathematics at the Royal Indian Engineering mes Cooper’s Hill ; AND JOHN BORTHWICK DALE, M.A., Assistant Professor of Mathematics at King’s College, London. Crown 8vo., cloth, 7s. 6d. net (post free 7s. 1od.). Five-Figure Tables of Mathematical Functions. Comprising Tables of Logarithms, Powers of Numbers, Trigonometric, Elliptic, and other Transcendental Functions. By JOHN BORTHWICK DALE, M.A., Assistant Professor of Mathematics at King’s College, London. vi+92 pages. Demy 8vo., cloth, 3s. 6d. net (post free 3s. 1od.). This collection of Tables has been selected for use in the examinations of the University of London. Logarithmic and Trigonometric Tables (To Five Places of Decimals) By JOHN BORTHWICK DALE, M.A., Assistant Professor of Mathematics at King’s College, London. Demy 8vo., cloth, 2s. net (post free 2s. 3d.). Traverse Tables. With an Introductory Chapter on Co-ordinate Surveying. By HENRY LOUIS, M.A., Professor of Mining and Lecturer on Surveying, Armstrong College, New- castle-on-Tyne ; and G. W. CAUNT, M.A., Lecturer in Mathematics, Armstrong College, Newcastle-on- Tyne. XXvill + 92 pages. Demy 8vo., flexible cloth, rounded corners, 4s. 6d net (post free 4s. 9d.). ’ ‘ aie ? ct a -_— ? As ai Fj — 4 Technical and Scientific Publications — 5 By JULIUS B. COHEN, PuH.D., B.Sc., Professor of Organic Chemistry in the University of Leeds, and Associate of Owens é College, Manchester. Demy 8vo., cloth, 21s. net (post free 21s. 6d.). The book is written for students who have already completed an elementary course of Organic Chemistry, and is intended largely to take the place of the advanced text-book. For it has long been the opinion of the author that, when the principles of classification and synthesis and the properties of fundamental groups have been acquired, the object of the teacher should be, not to multiply facts of a similar kind, but rather to present to the student a broad and general outline of the more important branches of the subject. This method of treatment, whilst it avoids the dictionary arrange- ment which the text-book requires, leaves the writer the free disposal of his materials, so that he can bring together related substances, irrespective of their nature, and deal thoroughly with important theoretical questions which are often inadequately treated in the text-book. The Chemical Synthesis of Vital Products and the Inter-relations between Organic Compounds. BY RATHAEL MELDOLA,. FR, V:P:C.5.50.0G.. ete, Professor of Chemistry in the City and Guilds of London Technical College, Finsbury. Vol. I., xvit+338 pages. Super Royal 8vo., cloth, 21s. net (post free 21s. 5d.). The great achievements of modern Organic Chemistry in the domain of the synthesis or artificial production of compounds which are known to be formed as the result of the vital activities of plants and animals have not of late years been systematically recorded. The object of the present book is to set forth a statement, as complete as possible, of the existing state of knowledge in this most important branch of science. The Chemistry of the Diazo-Compounds. By JOHN CANNELL CAIN, D.Sc. (Manchester and Tibingen), Editor of the Publications of the Chemical Society. 176 pages. Demy 8vo., Ios. 6d. net (post free ros. 1od.). Lectures on Theoreticaland Physical Chemistry. By Dr. J. H. VAN ’T HOFF, Professor of Chemistry at the University of Berlin. Translated by R. A. LEHFELDT, D.Sc, Professor of Physics at the Transvaal Technical Institute, Johannesburg. In three volumes, demy 8vo., cloth, 28s. net (post free 28s. 7d.), or separately as follows : PARTI. CHEMICAL DYNAMICS. 254 pages, with 63 Illustra- tions. 12s. net (post free 12s. 5d.). PaRT I]. CHEMICAL STATICS. 156 pages, with 33 Illustrations. 8s. 6d. net (post free 8s. 1od.). PART III. RELATIONS BETWEEN PROPERTIES AND COMPOSITION. 143 pages, 7s. 6d. net (post free 7s. 10d.) on with the - _Experimental Researches. Furnace. Membre de I’Institut ; Professor of Chemistry at the Sorbonne. AUTHORIZED ENGLISH EDITION. _ ’ Translated by A. T. de MOUILPIED, M.Sc., Ph.D., on Assistant Lecturer in Chemistry in the University of Liverpool. xii+ 307 pages, with Illustrations. Demy $8vo., cloth, Ios. 6d. net (post free 10s. 10d.). j “There is hardly a page of it which is not crowded with interest, and hardly a section which does not teem with suggestion ; and if the coming of this English edition of the book has been so long delayed, we may still be thankful that it has come at last, and come in a form which it is a pleasure to handle and a delight to read,” —/ectrical Review, Electrolytic Preparations. Exercises for use in the Laboratory by Chemists and Electro-Chemists. 3 By Dr. KARL ELBS, Professor of Organic and Physical Chemistry at the University of Giessen. Translated by R. S. HUTTON, M.Sc., Demonstrator and Lecturer on Electro-Chemistry at the University of Manchester. xii+100 pages. Demy 8vo., cloth, 4s. 6d. net (post free 4s. 1od.). The book contains a complete course of examples on the application of electrolysis to the preparation of both inorganic and organic sub- | stances. It will be found useful as filling a distinct gap in the text- book literature suitable for use in chemical laboratories, and should enable the chemist to make use of the many valuable and elegant methods of preparation which have been worked out during recent years, the advantages and ease of application of which he cannot appreciate without such a guide. Introduction to Metallurgical Chemistry for Technical Students. By J. H.. STANSBIE, .B-Sc..(LOND.),..F..1.G., Associate of Mason University College, and Lecturer in the Birmingham University Technical School. SECOND EDITION. xli+252 pages. Crown 8vo., cloth, 4s. 6d. An Experimental Course of Chemistry for Agri- cultural Students. By T. S. Dymonp, F.I.C., Lately Principal Lecturer in the Agricultural Department, County Technical Laboratories, Chelmsford. New Impression. 192 pages, with 50 Illustrations. Crown 8vo., cloth, 2s. 6d. A History of Chemistry. By Dr. HUGO BAUER, Royal Technical Institute, Stuttgart. Translated by R. V. STANFORD, B.Sc. (LOND.), Priestley Research Scholar in the University of Birmingham. Crown 8vo., cloth, 3s. 6d. net. - By HENRI MOISSAN, | — is da . STRUTT, F.R.S., . — + Fellow of Trinity College, Cambridge. _ SECOND EDITION, REVISED AND ENLARGED. : bd ‘ . . ___—_viii++222 pages, with Diagrams. Demy 8vo., cloth, 8s. 6d. net | (post free 8s. Iod.). nf -_ ‘Tf only a few more books of this type were written, there might be some hope of a general appreciation of the methods, aims, and results of science, which would go far to promote its study. . . . A book for which no praise can be excessive.” — _ Atheneum. _ Astronomical Discovery. By HERBERT HALL TURNER, D.Sc, F.R.S., Savilian Professor of Astronomy in the University of Oxford. xli+225 pages, with Plates and Diagrams. Demy 8vo., cloth, ros. 6d. net . (post free Ios. 11d.). An Introduction to the Theory of Optics. By ARTHUR SCHUSTER, Ph.D., Sc.D., F.R.S., Recently Professor of Physics at the University of Manchester. xvi+ 340 pages, with Illustrations. Demy 8vo., cloth, 15s. net (post free 15s. 5d.). ‘We know of no book written with a set purpose better adapted to serve the purpose for which it was written, nor any that the earnest student of optics will find more interesting and profitable. The work itself, without the confession of the preface, shows that Professor Schuster is a teacher, and every page bears evidence that he is a master of his subject. ... We heartily recommend the book to our readers.’”’— Ophthalmic Review. Wood. A Manual of the Natural History and Industrial Applications of the Timbers of Commerce. BY GC: S-BOULGER FF LS- FCS. A6 1) Professor of Botany and Lecturer on Forestry in the City of London College, and formerly in the Royal Agricultural College. NEW EDITION. Revised and Enlarged and profusely illustrated. Demy 8vo., 12s. 6d. net (post free 12s. 11d.). “It is just the book that has long been wanted by land agents, foresters, and wood- men, and it should find a place in all technical school libraries.” —Fie/d. Manual of Alcoholic Fermentation and the Allied Industries. By CHARLES G. MATTHEWS, F.L.C., F.C.S., ETC. xvi+295 pages, with 8 Plates and 40 Illustrations. Crown 8vo., cloth, 7s. 6d. net (post free 7s. 10d.). ; “This is a book worthy of its author, and well worth perusing by every student. a . . . The student, both old and young, as well as the practical brewer, will find this 7 book gives him some very useful information.”—Brewers’' Guardian. The Evolution Theory. By D: MANN, Professor of Zoology in the University of | Translated, with the Author’s co-operation, by J. ARTHUR TH _. Regius Professor of Natural History in the University of Aberdeen MARGARET THOMSON. Two vols., xvi+ 416 and viii + 396 pages, wi 130 Illustrations. Royal 8vo., cloth, 32s. net. . ‘« The subject has never been so fully and comprehensively expounded before ; and i ca and the absorbing interest of his exposition, with its prodigious wealth of illustration, its vast store of zoological knowledge, its ingenious interpretations and far-reaching _ it is not necessary to subscribe to all the author’s tenets in order to recognise the a theories. English readers have reason to be grateful to Professor and Mrs. Thomson © for their admirable translation.” — 7he Times. The Chances of Death and Other Studies in Evolution. By Kart Prarson, M.A., F.R.S., Professor of Applied Mathematics in University College, London, and formerly Fellow of King’s College, Cambridge. 2 vols., xii + 388 and 460 pages, with numerous Illustrations. Demy 8vo., cloth, 25s. net (post free 25s. 6d.). The Life of the Salmon. With reference more especially to the Fish in Scotland. By W. L. CALDERWOOD, F.R.S.E., Inspector of Salmon Fisheries for Scotland. Illustrated. Demy $vo., 7s. 6d. net.” - « We have no hesitation whatever in advising al] persons interested in the salmon, whether as fishermen, naturalists, or legislators, to add this book to their libraries.”’ —Nature. Animal Behaviour. By Professor C. Ltioyp MorcGan, LL.D., F.R.S., Principal of University College, Bristol. vili+ 344 pages, with 26 Illustrations. Crown 8vo., cloth, 7s. 6d. net (post free 7s. I1d.). This important contribution to the fascinating subject of animal psycho- logy covers the whole ground from the behaviour of cells up to that of the most highly developed animals. BY THE SAME AUTHOR. . Habit and Instinct. viii+ 352 pages, with Photo- gravure Frontispiece. Demy 8vo., cloth, 16s. Professor ALFRED RUSSEL WALLACE :—‘“‘ An admirable introduction to the study of a most important and fascinating branch of biology, now for the first time based upon a substantial foundation of carefully observed facts and logical induction from oo BY THE SAME AUTHOR. : The Springs of Conduct. Cheaper Edition. viii+317 pages. Large crown 8vo., cloth, 3s. 6d. This volume deals with the Source and Limits of Knowledge, the Study of Nature, the Evolution of Scientific Knowledge, Body, and Mind, Choice, Feeling, and Conduct. ' BY THE SAME AUTHOR. Psychology for Teachers. New Edition, entirely rewritten. xii+308 pages. Crown 8vo., cloth, 4s. 6d. An Introduction to Child Study. By Dr. W. B. DRUMMOND. Crown 8vo., cloth, 6s. net. The Child’s Mind: Its Growth and Training. By W. E. Urwick, University of Leeds. Crown 8vo., cloth, 4s. 6d. net. LONDON: EDWARD ARNOLD, 41 & 43 MADDOX STREET, W. oe SS er yee me ats, nder. rey ‘2 Author Hill ,..ilex The body at work. Title ‘ THIS : me] * & ‘ # A? he ES pe ae : at - wes ort + "4 eects 2 a ~ 7. % — mA pode B75) | x a ms i Se - ar aoe ‘Acme Library Card Pocket Under Pat. ‘Ref. Index File” fe _-Made by LIBRARY BUREAU * it sens UhasaT dicueeen a reeset net eae aE : ans : - e = Ds $0- 0h 4-HOUOR aha btueer ee area ee EN Sop Sette tint ie radia ltd ete ee es ene ey OT TOP terete 4. ee > < irae or sane td ~ -Peepbhyeemuyeen wien eit te ee $e Gri naan ean go " : a tr Se. — . Spfaweeae — - = = mie - E rate iol 96 x + EL eerar ees Markati ns else ee Sub te ae ae (ghrp i Tag te Ne. tel Te ~~ fb contd Peel noe ot - 2 si 3 ‘ j Dts Rtg Sic ER ae A ere uaes a le tat fea ah oh 4 boa a ATi on a Oe SU tat lo ae aerate EAN ay : A Sc Nc a RT SL Pe SrhLaas Sogo ree en ee amet t tae Sirieem sane eee at : ——— ee rae een vachinetien ee ee ‘my bees aie =A Sate cen mere rm oh : oon ow ret era rm muuiaerwvarestraseeavansmmoeee - metiseesomnets = ew sr pores | a seeetneeritenrnca gece, morte te Neneh petite sisemenane toe i wap iebeaiipetionan eo fame tee. -cmnmntenndenenalie nnerpronwrsangenecete rae ~smee 4 oe avaamerer ae mmr | z ate poe cars Pare 5 { ene sAt AT ini vies patpeceeeinnes eaeebennipemicnre ee = epnanemnnerennines nnenoeegiriaeen ne =a ome ne a re 5 = 7 : tenet ate oiy, sac hgwree™s all a rere Pov — T ri nares peoeomenpeenpoeneteenpe == : a i SA eS doo rr ae —— LE NE Se memmenyrtnystorintonprmemenenndusatentshennteermnonnnner Pniemererneee | =i : ah ee pees ch ae - mer = = . ~“F avcp—atmerronipaqeenonen : me j wre m——omprnmiprenrnnenenponn ee a ene ees ‘ — =-oe . ! Pee ree eememnaraneneseece hy : lait ps nore nadine ete beae 5 : jantcimiateame sine eee i "ss Pruner wrens epee ene eneasenr eam aprons et -teeneenyaanntanieenmmnentemen tient ‘ ane am Pes OTOL I ich PAE Core ea Stes, an z . a 4 i ‘ PEP TAEITT fos ee " " — =- Fr 7—w—ninaytananninntinyestigmaataebnjpmmainntngumenent Ley Nar pennaraehe haewanetapcminmee eae eer RN a vernowemesteeereene ee TT + —— = : oa em we oe nthe bce Bimneshansonnianhmnata Beato med ieres a eeah ea IRR tt et a sgt Pt er rarer ~* abe _