BREVIORA Museum of Comparative Zoology Mths etesess,,, — A) 111 el = US ISSN 0006-9698 CAMBRIDGE, MASss. 10 JuLty 2013 NUMBER 534 SIZE-ASSORTATIVE PAIRING AND SOCIAL MONOGAMY IN A NEOTROPICAL LIZARD, ANOLIS LIMIFRONS (SQUAMATA: POLYCHROTIDAE) ALEXIS HARRISON ApstrActT. Social monogamy, the formation of stable male-female pairs, is uncommon among reptiles and is particularly rare among squamates, in which only a handful of cases has been reported. Only one case of persistent pair formation has ever been reported 1n anoles, for Anolis limifrons, at a single site in Costa Rica. Detailed studies of A. limifrons at other sites, however, have not shown evidence of pair formation. I revisited the site where pairing was originally reported to observe pair behavior in this species and to measure morphological traits of paired and unpaired animals. I confirmed that male-female pairs are commonly encountered in the wild, although a smaller proportion of the animals observed in this study were found in pairs than previously reported. I also found evidence for size-assortative pairing; larger males tended to be found with larger females and smaller males were found with smaller females. I did not find any differences in the morphology of paired and unpaired animals. Although social monogamy has not been widely reported in squamates, I suggest that more examples of this phenomenon will be described as the social behaviors of poorly known species are increasingly subject to study. Key worps: Anolis limifrons; mating system; assortative mating; pair formation; monogamy Social monogamy, the persistent associa- tion between an adult male and an adult female during the breeding season, is rela- tively common among birds and mammals (Wittenberger and Tilson, 1980), although genetic or mating monogamy is more rare (Petrie and Kempenaers, 1998). Among reptiles however, social monogamy and pair formation are very rare (Uller and Olsson, Museum of Comparative Zoology—Department of Herpetology, Harvard University, 26 Oxford Street, Cam- bridge, Massachusetts 02138, U.S.A.; e-mail: asharris@ fas.harvard.edu 2008), and genetic fidelity is almost un- known. Squamates in particular are usually both socially and genetically promiscuous (Bull, 2000; Uller and Olsson, 2008). There are a few notable exceptions to this generalization (Bull, 2000). The best known case is that of the sleepy lizard, Tiliqua rugosa, a long-lived skink native to southern and central Australia (Bull, 2000). This species is active through spring and early summer, when it forages on vegetation, eggs, nestlings, and carrion across a broad home range that can overlap with the home range © The President and Fellows of Harvard College 2013. 2 BREVIORA of other individuals of both sexes. Individu- als form stable pairs that appear to be socially and genetically monogamous, both within a single breeding season and across multiple breeding seasons (Bull, 2000). Paired animals are often observed in close proximity to each other, and when they are experimentally separated, both males and females actively work to relocate their partners using chemosensory cues (Bull ef al., 1993). Pairs of animals can persist over several breeding seasons; one pair was observed together over 10 consecutive years (Bull, 1994; Bull et al, 1998). However, even in this species not all animals are observed exclusively in pairs: 40% of females and 18% of males were found with more than one partner in a season during radio-tracking surveys (Bull et a/., 1998). It appears that males that pursue a polygynous strategy do not benefit: females paired with polygamous males were more likely to have multiple- paternity litters, as revealed by microsatellite paternity analyses (Bull et al, 1998). It is unclear whether pair formation is equally beneficial to females and, if so, how. Australian skinks in the genus Egernia have also been shown to form socially monogamous pairs in nature (E. stokesii: Gardner et al., 2002; E. cunninghami: Stow and Sunnucks, 2004; E. whitii: Chapple and Keogh, 2005). In Egernia saxatilis, not only do males and females form monogamous pairs, they live in close proximity to their subadult offspring in a situation that mirrors the “‘nuclear family” found in other verte- brates (O'Connor and Shine, 2003). The Tasmanian snow skink, Niveoscincus micro- lepidotus, also forms pairs that persist, on average, for 29 days during the breeding season (Olsson and Shine, 1998). Aside from skinks, at least two species of Chameleon, Chamaeleo hoehnelii and C. jacksoni, have also been observed in stable pairs in the field (Toxopeus et al., 1988). In C. hoehnelii, pairs No. 534 persisted for an average of 85 days, and 30- 40% of all animals were observed in pairs. In C. jacksoni, pairs persisted for an average of 63 days. About half of females were observed in pairs, whereas about a third of males were paired. Despite these reports of pairing behavior in a handful of species, social monogamy is thought to be extremely rare in squamates. Numerous studies of a wide variety of species support the notion that most squamates are polygamous (reviewed in Stamps, 1983). Why Monogamy? Little is known about why some species of lizards associate in pairs while most do not (Bull, 2000). Three non—mutually exclusive hypotheses may account for social monoga- my across animal taxa: (1) males remain with females to provide parental care (e.g., food or protection); (2) males guard females because the potential benefit of matings with other females is outweighed by the loss of paternity if other males mate with the focal female; and (3) an individual may remain in the presence of a mate because it benefits directly from their presence (e.g., male may fend off harassment by other males, or both partners may improve the chance of spotting predators; Bull, 2000). Parental care is typically rudimentary or absent in squamates (Gans, 1996); therefore, parental care is unlikely to explain most examples of social monogamy in lizards. A more likely expla- nation 1s that either the males, the females, or both directly benefit from pairing with a single partner. One plausible scenario is that monogamy evolves as a consequence of mate guarding in species where it is difficult for males to successfully defend multiple fe- males, such as when females occur at low densities and occupy a broad home range (Emlen and Oring, 1977; Bull, 2000). Alter- natively, social monogamy may evolve when 2013 the direct benefits of pairing are especially high, such as when predation risk is substan- tially reduced by an extra set of eyes. When monogamy does evolve, the process by which individuals form pairs becomes highly important because an_ individual’s fitness may be closely tied to the quality of its mate. Each individual should therefore strive to pair with the highest quality mates. A common pattern in animals is for the largest males to pair with the largest females and smaller males to pair with smaller females, a pattern known as size-assortative pairing (SAP; Crespi, 1989). SAP can be a product of three processes: mutual mate emorce® for large “body ‘size*(e.g:, beetles: Harari et al, 1999; spiders: Masumoto, 1999); physical constraints (e.g., beetles: Brown, 1993; fish: Bisazza, 1997); or mate availability—when, for some reason, indi- viduals that are similar in size are more likely to encounter each other and pair by chance (e.g., limpets: Pal et al., 2006). One way that mate availability could lead to a pattern of size assortative mating would be that, on reaching sexual maturity, an animal pairs with the first unpaired, sexually receptive animal they encounter. Older and larger animals are likely to be paired already and smaller animals are not yet sexually mature, so they are most likely to pair with an animal of similar size. It may be possible to differentiate among these processes based on differences between paired and unpaired animals. For example, if one sex is more abundant than the other, paired animals should be larger than un- paired animals in the more abundant sex if mutual mate choice for body size is operat- ing. In contrast, if physical constraints are responsible for SAP, then the size of unpaired animals should be related to the size distribution of potential mates—large animals may remain unpaired if large mates are rare. Studying SAP can therefore provide LIZARD ASSORTATIVE MATING 3 insight onto the process of pair formation. In addition, when SAP occurs, it can also have important implications for social behavior, population genetics, and even, potentially, speciation if assortative mating produces a division in the gene pool (Crespi, 1989; Kawecki, 1997; Nagel and Schluter, 1998: Harari et al, 1999: Bessa-Gomes et al., 2003). Pairing Behavior in A. limifrons A surprising candidate for pair formation and SAP in lizards is a Costa Rican population of Anolis limifrons, a slender arboreal lizard that is abundant in a variety of habitats from southern Mexico to Panama (Savage, 2002). This is very unusual behavior for an anole, a genus in which males typically gain access to multiple females by defending a territory and excluding other males (Tri- vers, 1976; Andrews, 1985; Jenssen and Nunez, 1998; Losos, 2009). Pair formation in A. limifrons was first reported by Talbot (1979), who found that 70% of adults were found in male-female pairs; that is, a single male and a single female were found within 2 m of each other with no other lizards present. Mark-recap- ture data showed that these pairs persisted for 4-6 months, approximately the adult lifespan for this species. Detailed observa- tions of pair behavior revealed that individ- uals in these pairs display to each other frequently (Fig. 1) and move in tandem for distances up to 20 m (Talbot, 1979). Stable pairs have not been observed in other populations of A. Jlimifrons despite extensive study; in fact, other populations demonstrate typical resource-defense polyg- yny (Andrews and Rand, 1983; Andrews and Stamps, 1994). Pair formation has also never been studied in other anoles, though it has been suggested for two species beside A. limifrons on the basis of the proximity of 4 BREVIORA A male A. limifrons displays to a female Figure 1. on an adjacent perch. These animals were observed in close proximity and were interacting. No other lizards were observed in the area, suggesting that these animals form a pair. sleeping males and females (A. occultus: Gorman, 1980; A. cuvieri: Rios-Lopez and Puente-Colon, 2007). This study had three objectives: to observe pairs of A. limifrons at the site where Talbot collected his data, to determine if such pairs demonstrate size- assortative pairing, and to compare the morphology of paired and unpaired animals. MATERIALS AND METHODS Field Data Field data were collected during a 17-day period from 19 April to 5 May 2007 at La Selva Biological Reserve in the Heredia province of Costa Rica between 7:30am and 5:30pm. Individual A. /imifrons were spotted by walking slowly along established trails while visually surveying vegetation, a standard method for conducting herpetolog- ical surveys (Doan, 2003). When an individ- No. 534 ual was spotted, two observers positioned themselves approximately 5 m away from the subject. One observer recorded the behavior of the focal animal while the other scanned the area to identify other nearby individuals. If no additional lizards or displays were observed during the first 15 minutes of observation, the lizard was considered “‘sol- itary’ and was captured. If the initial lizard displayed, or if another lizard was spotted nearby, the observation was extended to 30 minutes. Following the procedure of Talbot (1979), two lizards were considered a pair if they were observed within 2 m of each other, and no other lizards were seen within 5 m during the observation period. In most cases, paired males and females were less than half a meter apart. On some occasions, more than one lizard was ob- served within 2 m of the initial animal. In all of these cases, a single female was found in the presence of multiple males; multiple females were never found in such a group. It seems likely that these groups represented a pair and one or more intruders but, because it was impossible to determine which male or males were intruding, these cases were excluded from the final analysis (but see supplementary materials to see how the inclusion of these animals would affect the observed pattern). In some cases, more than one individual was identified during an observation, but not all lizards were success- fully captured. If a lizard was spotted but not captured, no animals from the observation were included in subsequent analysis. After 30 minutes of observation, attempts were made to capture all lizards by hand or by noose. Each lizard captured was mea- sured, photographed with the dewlap ex- tended, marked with a unique pattern of colored ink dots on the ventral surface, and released at the site of capture. The data recorded for each individual were sex, snout- vent length (svl, a standard variable for 2013 estimating body size in lizards), forelimb length, hindlimb length, tail length, and the length, depth and width of the head, to the nearest tenth of a millimeter. Males less than 32 mm svl and females less than 35 mm svl were considered juveniles (following Talbot 1979) and were excluded from further analysis (but see supplementary materials to see how inclusion of these animals would affect the analysis). All measurements were taken with digital calipers by the same individual. The surface area of the dewlap was measured for all males in ImageJ (Abramoff et al, 2004) and scaled by reference to a l-cm grid. Statistical Analysis The morphological traits of paired and solitary animals were compared using an analysis of covariance. These traits included svl, fore- and hindlimb length, tail length, head length, width and depth, and dewlap area; body size was included as a covariate in the analysis of all traits except body size. Males and females were treated separately. Because no tests approached significance, no correction for multiple tests was used. The correlation between body size for members of a pair was calculated using Pearson’s correlation coefficient. All statis- tics were calculated in SPSS. RESULTS Among 150 animals observed and cap- tured, 40 individuals were found in male- female pairs; 33 males and 15 females were observed alone; 22 males and 8 females were observed in groups of more than two animals; and 22 males were observed in male-male pairs. Ten males were observed with another animal, but the other individual was not captured and the sex could not be definitely assigned. When animals whose pair status could not be assigned were LIZARD ASSORTATIVE MATING 3 excluded (e.g., animals in groups, male-male pairs, pairs where one animal was not captured), 57.1% of females and 37.7% of males were observed in pairs (44.9% average for both sexes). Individuals in male-female pairs were often interacting during the time that they were observed, although copulation was never witnessed. Visual displays were performed by both males and females (males displayed in 8/20 observations, whereas females displayed in 2/20). Often one animal, typically the male, followed the other up and down a perch, and from one perch to another (5/20 observations), often moving slowly and frequently stopping until the partner was quite close. During the course of three observations, a male and female in close proximity were approached by a second male, who the first male proceeded to chase away. While these males were thus distracted, a third male would suddenly appear and approach the female while displaying. In one case, the female approached the third male and watched him display. In the other two cases, the female retreated from the third male. Copulation was never observed during the course of these intrusions. Male and female body size were positively correlated for paired animals (Fig. 2; R = 0.50, P = 0.039), and these results were qualitatively similar when smaller animals were included or when animals found in groups with one female and several males were included (see supplementary materials); however, neither body size (Fig. 2), body dimensions, or dewlap area differed between paired and solitary animals (see supplemen- tary materials). DISCUSSION Pair formation and social monogamy are rarely observed in reptiles, particularly squa- mates; yet, during the course of this study, 6 BREVIORA 40 5 39 - A a7 E | E > Yn 36 ° re oO = 35 | 34 - é 33 Z Up at T = T 1 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 Female SVL (mm) Figure 2. Correlation between male and female body size for paired animals. Each pair is symbolized by a grey diamond. male-female pairs were observed quite fre- quently. In most cases, members of a pair were within 50 cm or less of each other and were frequently observed displaying to each other. Pairs also showed coordinated move- ment, in which one animal (usually the male) followed the female vertically or horizontally through the habitat. These observations, combined with Talbot’s (1979) observations of pair persistence over 4-6 months, indicate that these animals are not in association by chance—they are in fact associating in pairs. This social behavior is highly atypical for lizards in general and for anoles in particular, which more typically demonstrate resource- defense polygyny (Losos, 2009). This behav- 1or may also be atypical for A. /imifrons, which has been shown to defend polygynous territories in other parts of its range. These data also show one of the first reptilian examples of SAP (size-assortative pairing). We found no morphological differences between paired and unpaired animals, sug- gesting that mutual mate choice is an unlikely No. 534 explanation for SAP. Two other hypotheses to account for SAP, physical constraints and mate availability, were neither supported nor rejected by these data, but remain a fruitful avenue for future research efforts. Existence and Prevalence of Pairs The rates of pairing observed in this study were lower than those observed by Talbot: 45% vs. 70% of all observed individuals, respectively. The lower percentage in this study may have resulted because some paired individuals may have been observed when their partner was not visible, or pairs may have been excluded from analysis due to the presence of a temporary intruder. Unpaired animals might also have been mistaken for paired animals because of temporary prox- imity to a member of the opposite sex. It is also possible that the pairing behavior is not typical of A. limifrons and occurs occasionally at this location because of unknown environmental or social factors. In other well-studied populations of A. limifrons, pairing between males and females has never been reported, despite intensive study (Andrews and Rand, 1983; Andrews and Stamps, 1994). Rather, these popula- tions exhibit a sedentary polygamous mating system based on territory defense, a system typical of anoles. A possible explanation is that A. limifrons observed at sites outside La Selva actually belong to a different species, which has diverged in social behavior. Recent morphological work has suggested that A. limifrons might actually consist of three or more distinct species (Kohler and Sunyer, 2008). If A. limifrons at La Selva comprise a unique lineage, their distinct evolutionary history could have influenced the evolution of their social behavior in a number of ways, via shifts in habitat use, population density, or predation risk, to name a few possibilities. 2013 Another possibility is that A. /imifrons has a high degree of behavioral plasticity in their mating behavior and that local conditions determine what strategy they pursue. This would be consistent with the polygyny threshold model, which suggests that indi- vidual mating decisions may change when the distribution of resources shifts to allow higher densities or stable aggregations of females, or both, that males can monopolize (Altmann eft al, 1977). Although data on forest characteristics were not collected as part of this study, it appeared that pairs occur more frequently in primary forest, whereas clusters of animals were found in great abundance in disturbed habitat. This potential pattern could be a productive direction for future studies. Characteristics of Pairs Although the data show SAP in A. limifrons, 1 did not find support for the mutual mate choice hypothesis, the physical constraints hypothesis, or the mate availabil- ity hypothesis. Paired and unpaired animals did not differ in any of the morphological variables that were considered, including body size; head, limb, and tail dimensions; or dewlap area, suggesting that mate choice is not operating on these traits (see supple- mentary materials for details). Mutual mate choice may be operating on traits that we did not consider, or a pattern of mate choice in the traits that were examined could have been obscured by the shortcomings of our survey methods or confounding ecological processes that also act on body size, such as differential mortality. Mating constraints seem unlikely to be responsible for SAM in A. limifrons because pairs were observed that were somewhat size- mismatched (and that deviate substantially from the observed correlation), but I cannot rule out this possibility entirely because LIZARD ASSORTATIVE MATING i) physical constraints may only be relevant to the most extreme mismatches, and I did not observe all possible size combinations in the population. Captive breeding experiments with highly size-mismatched individuals could be conducted to test this hypothesis explicitly. Likewise, no spatial or temporal discontinuity was observed in the distribu- tion of size classes that would support a mate availability mechanism as an explanation for SAM. However, the spatial distribution of body sizes was not explicitly examined, and the duration of this study was insufficient to uncover temporal patterns of variation in body size. Further studies that explicitly examine the spatial and temporal distribu- tion of body size of paired and unpaired animals may clarify this issue. In short, the process that is responsible for the pattern of SAP in A. limifrons remains obscure; uncov- ering the process of pair formation in this species will require substantial future efforts. More Questions than Answers The existence of socially monogamous pairs in A. limifrons does not necessarily imply that these pairs are also genetically monogamous. Molecular studies of parent- age will be necessary to characterize the genetics of this unusual social behavior. Moreover, we still have little conception of how pairs are formed in the wild or what benefits accrue to pair members. There is a wealth of opportunity for future studies in the field and in the lab on these questions. The unusual behavior of A. /imifrons also begs the question: Could social monogamy be more common in lizards than previously thought? The behavior of most species of anole is poorly known from observations in the field; indeed, the behavior of very few lizard species has been studied in the wild. Species that have been overlooked in previ- ous studies are precisely the ones that are 8 BREVIORA predicted to exhibit social or genetic monog- amy; for example species with wide home ranges without established territories or terri- torial behavior, low densities, and cryptic appearance and habits (Emlen and Oring, 1977). The unusual social behavior of A. limifrons described in this study, combined with the fact that it is relatively abundant, easily observed, and geographically widespread, could make this a useful species for future studies of pairing behavior in squamates. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS Field assistance was provided by the Reverend P. Humphrey, M. D., and C. Tongier. Comments on the manuscript were kindly provided by D. L. Mahler, E. H. Kay, Y. Stuart, J. Losos, and two anonymous reviewers. This work was funded in part with a grant from the Organization for Tropical Studies (OTS). Permission to work on A. limifrons came from OTS and the Minesterio del Ambiente y Energia (MINAE) of Costa Rica. This work was authorized under Har- vard University Animal Care Protocol 26-11. LITERATURE CITED ABRAMOFF, M. D., P. J. MAGELHAES, AND S. J. Ram. 2004. Image processing with ImageJ. Biophotonics International 11(7): 36-42. ALTMANN, S. A., S.S. WAGNER, AND S. LENINGTON. 1977. Two models for the evolution of polygyny. Behav- ioral Ecology and Sociobiology 2: 397-410. ANDREwsS, R. M. 1985. Mate choice by females of the lizard, Anolis carolinensis. Journal of Herpetology 19: 284-289. ANpREws, R. M., AND A. S. RANb. 1983. Limited dispersal of juvenile Anolis limifrons. Copeia 1983: 429-434. Anprews, R. M., AND J. A. Stamps. 1994. Temporal variation in sexual size dimorphism of Anolis limifrons in Panama. Copeia 1994: 613-622. BessA-Gome_s, C., J. CLOBERT, S. LEGENDRE, AND A. P. Mo .ter. 2003. Modelling mating patterns given mutual mate choice: the importance of individual mating preferences and mating system. Journal of Biological Systems 11: 205-219. No. 534 Bisazza, A. 1997. Sexual selection constrained by internal fertilization in the livebearing fish Xenotoca eiseni. Animal Behavior 54: 1347-1355. Brown, W. D. 1993. The cause of size-assortative mating in the leaf beetle Trirhabda canadensis. Behavioral Ecology and_ Sociobiology 33(3): 151-157. BuLL, C. M. 1994. Population dynamics and pair fidelity in sleepy lizards, pp. 159-174. In L. J. Vitt, and E. R. Pianka eds. Lizard Ecology: Historical and Experimental Perspectives. Princeton, New Jersey, Princeton University Press. BuL._, C. M. 2000. Monogamy in lizards. Behavioral Processes 51: 7-20. BuLL, C. M., G. S. BEDFoRD, AND B. A. SCHULTz. 1993. How do sleepy lizards find each other? Herpetolo- gica 49: 294-300. BuLL, C. M., S. J. B. Cooper, AND B. C. BAGHUuRST. 1998. Social monogamy and extra-pair fertilization in an Australian lizard, Tiliqua rugosa. Behavioral Ecol- ogy and Sociobiology 44: 63-72. CHAPPLE, D. G., AND J. S. KeEoGH. 2005. Complex mating system and dispersal patterns in a social lizard, Egernia Molecular Ecology 14: 1215-1227. Crespl, B. J. 1989. Causes of assortative mating in arthropods. Animal Behavior 38: 980-1000. Doan, T. M. 2003. Which methods are most effective for surveying rain forest herpetofauna? Journal of Herpetology 37: 72-81. EMLEN, S. T., AND L. W. OrING. 1977. Ecology, sexual selection, and the evolution of mating systems. Science 197: 215-223. Gans, C. 1996. An overview of parental care among the Reptilia. Advances in the Study of Behavior 25: 145-157. GARDNER, M. G., C. M. BuLL, AND S. J. B. Cooper. 2002. High levels of genetic monogamy in the group-living Australian lizard Egernia_ stokesii. Molecular Ecology 11: 1787-1794. GorMAN, G. C. 1980. Anolis occultus, a small cryptic canopy lizard: are there pair bonds? Caribbean Journal of Science 15: 3-4. Harari, A. R., A. M. HANDLER, AND P. J. LANDOLT. 1999. Size-assortative mating, male choice, and female choice in the curculionid beetle Diaprepes abbreviatus. Animal Behavior 58: 1191-1200. JENSSEN, T. A., AND S. C. Nunez. 1998. Spatial and breeding relationships of the lizard, Anolis caroli- nensis: evidence of intrasexual selection. Behaviour 135: 981-1003. Kaweckl, T. J. 1997. Sympatric speciation via habitat specialization driven by deleterious mutations. Evolution 51: 1751-1763. whitil. 2013 KOHLER, G., AND J. SUNYER. 2008. Two new species of anoles formerly referred to as Anolis limifrons (Squamata: Polychrotidae). Herpetologica 64: 92-108. Losos, J. B. 2009. Lizards in an Evolutionary Tree Berkeley and Los Angeles, University of California Press. Masumoto, T. 1999. Size assortative mating and reproductive success in the funnel-web spider, Agelena limbata. Journal of Insect Biology 12: 353-361. NAGEL, L., AND D. SCHLUTER. 1998. Body size, natural selection, and speciation in sticklebacks. Evolution 52: 209-218. O’Connor, D., AND R. Sune. 2003. Lizards in ‘nuclear families’: a novel reptilian social system in Egernia saxatilis (Scincidae). Molecular Ecology 12: 743-752. Oxsson, M., AND R. SHINE. 1998. Chemosensory mate recognition may facilitate prolonged mate guarding by male snow skinks, Niveoscincus microlepidus. Behavioral Ecology and Sociobiology 43: 359-363. Pa, P., J. ERLANDSSON, AND M. SKoLp. 2006. Size- assortative mating and non-reciprocal copulation in a hermaphroditic intertidal limpet: test of the mate availability hypothesis. Marine Biology 148: 1273-1282. PeTriE, M., AND B. KEMPENAERS. 1998. Extra-pair paternity in birds: explaining variation between species and populations. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 13(2): 52-58. LIZARD ASSORTATIVE MATING 9 Rios-Lopez, N., AND A. R. PUENTE-COLON. 2007. Anolis cuvieri (Puerto Rico giant anole): reproductive ecology. Herpetological Review 38: 73-75. SAVAGE, J. M. 2002. The Amphibians and Reptiles of Costa Rica: A Herpetofauna between Two Conti- nents, between Two Seas Chicago, University of Chicago Press. STAMPS, J. A. 1983. Sexual selection, sexual dimorphism, and territoriality, pp. 169-204. In R. B. Huey, E. R. Pianka, and T. W. Schoener eds. Lizard Ecology: Studies of a Model Organism. Cambridge, Massa- chusetts, Harvard University Press. Stow, A. J., AND P. SuNNucks. 2004. High mate and site fidelity in Cunningham’s skinks (Egernia cunnin- hami) in natural and fragmented habitat. Molecular Ecology 13: 419-430. TALBoT, J. J. 1979. Time budget, niche overlap, inter- and intraspecific aggression in Anolis humilis and A. limifrons from Costa Rica. Copeia 1979: 472-481. Toxopeus, A. G., J. P. Kruyit, AND D. HILLENtus. 1988. Pair-bonding in chameleons. Naturwissenschaften 75: 268-269. Trivers, R. L. 1976. Sexual selection and resource- accruing abilities in Anolis garmani. Evolution 30: 253-269. ULLeR, T., AND M. Oxsson. 2008. Multiple paternity in reptiles: patterns and processes. Molecular Ecology 17: 2566-2580. WITTENBERGER, J. F., AND R. L. TiLson. 1980. The evolution of monogamy: hypotheses and evolution. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 11: 197-232. Note: Supplementary material referenced in this paper is available online at www.mcz.harvard.edu/Publications/. ™