Cornell University Library The original of this book is in the Cornell University Library. There are no known copyright restrictions in the United States on the use of the text. http://www.archive.org/details/cu31924003692435 69.11,866:67.99 - CONTRIBUTIONS FROM THE “ZOOLOGICAL LABORATORY OF THE MUSEUM oF COMPARATIVE ZOOLOGY. AT HARVARD COLLEGE. No, $21, ‘THE RELATION. OF PHOTOTROPISM . TO SW ARMING IN —THE. HONEY-BEE; APIS: MELLIFERA: L. By Dwiéu7 E. Minnicz. From THE Journat or PsycnonroLocy, Vor. I, No.2: CAMBEIDGE,, ‘MASS. iw. 8. A. ae pAPRIL, 1920. 59.11.856:57.99 CONTRIBUTIONS FROM THE ZOOLOGICAL LABORATORY OF THE MUSEUM OF COMPARATIVE ZOOLOGY AT HARVARD COLLEGE. No. 321. THE RELATION OF PHOTOTROPISM TO SWARMING IN THE HONEY-BEE, APIS MELLIFERA L. By Dwieut E. Minnicu. From rae JournaL or Psycuonioioey, Vou. II, No. 2. CAMBRIDGE, MASS., U.S.A. APRIL, 1920. H Reprinted from PsycHosroLogy, Vol. II, No. 2, April, 1920 THE RELATION OF PHOTOTROPISM TO SWARMING IN THE HONEY BEE, APIS MELLIFERA L.! DWIGHT E. MINNICH The honey bee is remarkable for the extent to which many of its activities are controlled by light. Observations and experi- ments demonstrating the strong photopositive responses of this animal have been detailed by Lubbock (’82, p. 278, 279, 284), Graber (’84) and Hess (13a, ’13b, 717). And indeed this feature of behavior must have been patent to many of the earlier workers, so strikingly and constantly is it exhibited. While conducting some experiments on the photic behavior of bees several years ago, I was greatly impressed by the strong, positive reaction to light which normal individuals almost invariably displayed. Bees in an active state of locomotion, or easily excited to such, exhibited an orientation which, for its rapidity of accomplishment and its accuracy of maintenance, was most spectacular. I say bees exhibiting vigorous locomo- tion, for, obviously, bees which are torpid and do not move freely—a condition frequently encountered during cool, damp weather—cannot show phototropism. Even the moribund con- dition seemed often to intensify rather than to weaken photic behavior, and bees scarcely able to creep were observed making a final struggle toward the light. My experiments were carried on well into the autumn, when it became more and more difficult to obtain bees in the field as the flowers became less abundant. I, therefore, installed a single comb of worker bees without queen, in a glass observation hive. The hive was kept darkened by means of small wooden covers fitted to the glass sides, and the exit was covered with a bit of screen wire to prevent the escape of the bees. Animals 1 Contributions from the Zodlogical Laboratory of the Museum of Comparative Zoélogy at Harvard College. No. 321. 177 178 DWIGHT E. MINNICH could be kept in this way in reasonably good condition for a month to six weeks, food being provided in case there was not sufficient in the comb. It was on these bees that I made the observations to be detailed in the present note, which I believe afford some evidence on the question of swarming and its rela- tion to light. The strong positive phototropism of individual bees is also characteristic of certain activities of the hive asa whole. Kellogg (03) was the first to call attention to the fact that swarming bees are strongly positive to light. He says (p. 694), ‘That the issuance from the hive at swarming time depends upon a sudden extra development of positive heliotropism seems obvious. The ecstasy comes and the bees crowd for the one spot of light in the normal hive, namely, the entrance opening. But when the covering jacket? is lifted and the light comes strongly in from above—my hive was under a skylight—they rush toward the top, that is toward the light. Jacket on and light shut off from above, down they rush; jacket off and light stronger from above than below and they respond like iron filings in front of an electro-magnet which has its current suddenly turned on.” These observations leave no doubt as to the strong positive photo- tropism of swarming bees. The statement, however, that the issuance of the swarm is dependent upon a “‘ sudden extra develop- ment” of positive phototropism has met with some difference of opinion. Thus von Buttel-Reepen has objected to this interpretation on the ground of a particular swarm instinct. He says (15, p. 168)“. . . . so ist in diesen Momenten [the emergence from the hive at swarming] nicht ein besonders starker Helio- tropismus die Ursache, sondern die Schwarminstinkte -dringen die Bienen in das Freie.” Another objection, and I believe a much more important one, is suggested by a statement of Hess (13 a, p.663). ‘‘Meine Versuche zeigen, dass die Neigung der Bienen, zum Hellen zu gehen, nicht auf die Zeit des Hochzeits- fluges beschrinkt ist.” This contention of Hess—that the * Kellogg had a glass observation hive covered with a black cloth jacket. RELATION OF PHOTOTROPISM TO SWARMING IN THE BEE 179 manifestation of photopositiveness is not confined to any given period, such as the nuptial flight, has been amply confirmed in my observations. I have already called attention to the fact that among individual bees an active animal exhibits, almost without exception, strong positive phototropism. Large aggre- gates of bees also show marked positive light reactions quite aside from swarm activities, as the following observations clearly show. The glass observation hive with its single comb of bees was situated at a south window where it was exposed to the sun during the morning hours. With the oncoming of the cool days of late October and early November, the bees became much less active. On bright days, however, the exposure to the warmth of the sun, and the corresponding rise of temperature within the hive, served to activate the colony until most of the bees were moving about the walls of the hive in a rapid uneasy manner. If at such a time the cover was removed from the end nearer the window, thus admitting the light, there was with the first penetrating rays, a sudden increase of the hum within. There- upon ensued a scene no less spectacular than that described by Kellogg (’03, p. 693). His words picture it exactly, ‘. the whole community of excited bees flowed—that is the word for it, so perfectly aligned and so evenly moving were all the individuals of the bee current—,” toward the illuminated side. A few hours later, however, when the sun had passed from the ‘hive and the temperature had fallen, particularly on the cooler -days, the uncovering of one end of the hive elicited no such response. With perhaps the exception of a very few individuals, the bees remained quietly in the swarm cluster. Nor was this failure to obtain a response a result of the lowered intensity of light, for the active worker bee responds to a fairly low intensity. It must, therefore, have been due to the difference in temperature. These facts, together with those previously stated, demon- strate conclusively that the pronounced phototropism, so con- spicuous in swarming bees, is not confined to the period of swarming. This condition of photosensitivity appears to remain fairly constant at all times in the animal, although active locomo- 180 DWIGHT E. MINNICH tion and the absence of other strong, distracting stimuli are necessary to its demonstration. The situation here differs from that described by Loeb (’90) for winged male and female ants, in which positive phototropism is confined exclusively to the period of the nuptial flight. We may agree with Kellogg, therefore, that the issuance of bees from the hive at swarming may be a simple exhibition of positive phototropism, but it is not the result of a sudden increase in phototropism. The sudden increase is in the activity of the animals. Consequently, the fundamental factors in swarm behavior are those which effect a condition of heightened ac- tivity, a condition in which locomotion is generally controlled by light. In my own observations the state of heightened activity doubtless arose from the conditions of temperature and the pre- vention of the bees from making their accustomed flights for defecation, etc. The factors which activate the swarm are not known. They may consist of particular swarm instincts, as von Buttel-Reepen (’15) has suggested, or they may even be more simple reflexes of as yet unknown nature. In any case, the eviderice seems clear that, although phototropism may be an important feature of swarm behavior, it is neither peculiar to this activity nor the primary causal agent of it. REFERENCES Burret-Regeren, H. von 1915 Leben und Wesen der Bienen. Braunschweig, 8vo, xiv + 300 pp. GraBER, V. 1884 Grundlinien zur Erforschung des Helligkeits-und Farben- sinnes der Tiere. Prag und Leipzig, 8vo, viii + 322 pp. Hess, C. 1913a Gesichtssinn. Handb. vergl. Physiol. von Hans Winterstein iv, 555-840. ; Huss, C. 1913b Experimentelle Untersuchungen tiber den angeblichen Farben- sinn der Bienen. Zool. Jahrb., xxxiv, Abt. allg. Zool. u. Physiol. der Tiere, 81-106. Hess, C. 1917 New experiments on the light reactions of plants and animals Jour. Animal Behavior, vii, 1-10. ; Kettoce, V. L. 1903 Some insect reflexes. Science, N. S., xviii, 693-696 Lozs, J. 1890 Der Heliotropismus der Thiere und seine Uebereinstimmun mit dem Heliotropismus der Pflanzen, Wurzburg, 8vo, 118 pp : Luspock, J. 1882 Ants, bees and wasps. New York, 12mo, xix + 448 pp CONTRIBUTIONS FROM THE ZOOLOGICAL LABORATORY OF THE MUSEUM OF COMPARATIVE ZOOLOGY AT HARVARD COLLEGE. ( Continued.) E. L. Marx, Detrector. *,* Abbreviations used :—. , B.M.C.Z.......... for Bull. Mus. Comp. Zou. P.A.A. «64.4.4. 4. for Proceed. Amer. Acad. Arts and Sci. P.B.S.N.H...... .. .« »for Proceed. Bost. Soc. Nat. Hist. Nors. — Copies of the List of Contributions, Numbere 1-276, wili be sent on application. 277. Wernrion, D. H.— Notes on the Reactions of Bivalve Mollusks to Changes in Light Intensity: Image Formation in Pecten. Jour. Anim. Behavior, 6 (4): 297-318. July-Aug., 1916. 278. Any, L. B.—The Influence of Light and Temperature upon the Migration o1 the Retinal Pigment of Planorbis trivolvis. Jour. Comp. Neurol., 26 (4) 859+ 389, 1 pl. Aug., 1916. 279. Waron, A. C.— Reactions of Paramoecium fandatads to Light. Jour. Anim. Behavior, @ (5) :335-340. Sept.-Oct., 1916. 280. Watron, A. C.— The ‘Refractive Body’ and the ‘Mitochondria’ of Ascaris canis Werner. Proc. Amer. Acad. Arts and Sci., 52 (5) : 253-266, 2 pls. Oct., 1916. : 281. Parker, G. H., anp Trrus, E.G. — The Structure of Metridium (Actinoloba marginatus Milne-Edwards with Special Reference to its Neuro-muscular ‘Mechanism. Jour. Exp. Zodl., 21 (4) :483-459,1 pl. Nov., 1916. 282. Parker, G. H.—The Effector fans of Actinians. Jour. Exp. Zodl., 21 (4): 461-484. Nov., 1916. 283. WaLtoNn, A. C.— A Case of the Occurrence of Ascaris isinuetee Schrank in Dogs. Jour. Parasitol., $ (1):39-41. Sept. [Nov.], 1916. 284. Parker, G. H. —Nervans Transmission in the Actinians. Jour. Exp. Zoul., 22 (1):87-94. Jan., 1917. 285. Parker, G. H.—The Movements of the Tentacles in Actinians. Jour. Exp Zool., 22 (1) :95-110. Jan., 1917. : 286. Parker, G. H.— Pedal Locomotion in Actinians. Jour. Exp. Zodl., 22 (1) :111- 124. Jan., 1917. 287. Parker, G. H.— The Responses of Hydroids to Gravity. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., 3 (2): 72-78. Feb., 1917. ‘ 288. Coz, W. H.—The Reactions of Drosophila ampelophila Loew to Gravity, Cen- trifugation, and Air Currents. Jour. Anim. Behavior, 7 (1) :71-80. Jan., 1917. 289. OLMSTED, J. M. D.— Geotropism in Planaria maculata. Jour. Anim. Behavior, 7 (1):81-86. Jan. 1917. _ 290. PARKER, G. H.— Actinian Behavior. Jour. Exp. Zoil., 22 (2) :193-229. Feb., 1917. 291. RepFiELD, E.S. P.—The Rhythmic. Contractions of the Mantle of Lamellibranchs. Jour. Exp. Zoél., 22 (2) : 231-239. ‘Feb., 1917. 292. Rep#FiEeLp, A. C. — The Reactions of the Melanophores of the Horned Toad. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., 3 (3) : 202-203. Mar., 1917. 293. REDFIELD, A.C. —The Codrdination of the Melanophore Reactions of the Horned Toad. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., 8 (3) : 204-205. Mar., 1917. 294. Pops, P. H.—The Introduction of West Indian Anurainto Bermuda, Bull. Mus. Comp. Zoél., 61 (6) :117-131, 2 pls. May, 1917. 295. Van Heusen, A. P.—The Skin of the Catfish (Amiurus nebulosus) as a Re- ceptive Organ for Light. Amer. Jour. Physiol., #@ (2) :212-214. Sept., 1917. 296. Parker, G,H., anp Van Heusen, A. P. — The Responses of the Catfish, Amiurus nebulosus, to Metallic and Non-metallic Rods. Amer, Jour. Physiol., 44 (3): 405-420. , Oct., 1917. 297. Parker, G.H.— The Pedal Locomotion of the Sea-Hare Aplysia californica. , dour. Exp. Zodl., 24 (1) :189-145. Oct., 1917. 298. PaRKER, G. H., anp Van HzvseEn, A. P.— The Reception of Mechanical Stimuli by the Skin, ‘Lateral: Line Organs and Ears in Fishes, especially i in Amiurus. Amer. Jour. Physiol., #4 (4) :463-489. Nov., 1917. 299. Parker, G. H. — The Power of Suction in the Sea-Anemone Cribrina. Jour. Exp. ZoGl., 2& (2) : 219-222. Nov., 1917. CONTRIBUTIONS FROM THE ZOOLOGICAL LABORATORY OF THE MUSEUM OF COMPARATIVE ZOOLOGY AT HARVARD COLLEGE. (Continued.) 300. Parker, G. H.—The Activities of Corymorpha. Jour. Exp. Zodl., 24 (2) :303- 331. Nov., 1917. 301. SrrinegR, C. E.—The Means of Locomotion in Planarians. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., B (12) :691-692. Dec., 1917. ; 302. Onmsrep,J.M. D.—The Regeneration of Triangular Pieces of Planaria maculata. A Study in Polarity. Jour. Exp. Zodl., 2 (1) :1657-176. Feb., 1918. ; 303. ; Hecut, S, —The Physiology of Ascidia atra Lesueur. I. General Physiology. 304. { II. Sensory Physiology. Jour. Exp. Zobl., 25 (1) :229-299. Feb., 1018. 305. Hxont,S.—Id. IIL. The Blood System. Amer.Jour. Physiol., 45 (3) : 167-187. Feb., 1918. 306. Bray, A. W. L.—The Reactions of the Melanophores of Amiurus to Light and Adrenalin. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., & (3) :58-60. Mar., 1918. 807. Watron, A. C.— The Odgenesis and Early Embryology of Ascaris canis Werner. Jour. Morvh., 3O (2) :527-603. Mar., 1918. 808, OLmsTED, J. M. D.— Experiments on the Nature of the Sense of Smell in the Common Catfish, Amiurus nebulosus (Lesueur). Amer. Jour. Physiol., £6 (5) 448-458, Aug., 1918. i 309. Ruprim.p, A. C.—The Physiology of the Melanophores ot the Horned Toad, Phrynosoma. . Jour. Exp. Zobl., 26 (2) :275-338, July, 1918. 310. Brooxs, E.S.— Reactions of Frogs to Heat and Cold. Amer. Jour. Physiol., 46 (5) 1498-501. Aug., 1918. 811. Cons, P. H.— Autonomous Responses of the Labial Palps of Anodonta. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., & (8) : 234-235. Aug., 1918. 312, Irwin, M.—The Nature of Sensory Stimulation by Salts. Amer. Jour. Physiol., A'? (2) :265-277. Nov., 1918. 313. Parker, G. H.—The Rate of Transmission in the Nerve Net of the Coelenterates. Jour. Gen. Physiol., 1 (2) :231-236. Nov., 1918. 314. Bienny, A. J.—The Effect of Adrenin on the Pigment Migration in the Melano- phores of the Skin and in the Pigment Cells of the Retina of the Frog. Jour. Exp. Zo@l., 27 (8): 391-396. Jan., 1919. 315. Jorpan, H.— Concerning Reissner’s Fiber in Teleosts. Jour. Comp. Neurol., 30 (2):217-227,1 pl. Feb., 1919. 316. Parker, G. H.—The Organization of Renilla. Jour. Exp. Zoél., 2'7 (4): 499- 607. Feb., 1919. ‘ 317. Hunt, H. R.— Regenerative Phenomena following the Removal of the Digestive Tube and the Nerve Cord of Earthworms. Bull. Mus. Comp. Zo6l., 62 (15): 569-581, 1 pl. Apr., 1919. / 318. Davis, D. W.— Asexual Multiplication and Regeneration in Sagartia luciae Verrill. Jour. Exp. Zodl., 28 (2):161-204, May, 1919. ; 319. ParRkER, G. H.—The Effects of the past Winter on the Occurrence of Sagartia luciae Verrill. Amer. Nat., 53 (626):280-281. May-June, 1919. 320. Munnicu, D. E.—The Photic Reactions of the Honey-Bee, Apis mellifera I. Jour. Exp. Zodl., 29 (3):3438-425. Nov., 1919. 821, Munnicu, D. E.—The Relation of Phototropism to Swarming in the Honey-Bee, Apis mellifera L. Jour. Psychobiol., 2 (2) :177-180. April, 1920, - CONTRIBUTIONS FROM” THE: ZOOLOGIOAL, LABORATORY OF | THE MUSEUM etiam iia tOOLOGY AT HARVARD — , _ CORUESE, (Cont ed. ee oe zB iL | Max, Directo! " » Abtivertolions aed ts = ~ BMQ.Z. 6 oe 0's soos for Ball, ites, Coun: 26a, . ; “PrAshs aaa Pope ee for’ Proceed. Amer, Acad: Ayts and Sei. 3 BBNE. set's oo oy for Proceed. Bost, Soc. Nat, Hist: i“ Nots.— Copii of the ‘List of: Oontribiutions,: umbere 1276, sot be Sg ant. ‘Wanton, D. Ha ; Changes in : Light - Intenai ay * = oration I fo Pocten, Toure. = Behsvior, 6 (a): 297-818." July-Aug., ot 218. _ Anny, LL.B The Influence of Light and te aperainee upon. the Pleat or ‘the Retinal Pigment of Planorbis | trivolvis. :BBOS _ 880, Tpl. Ang., tie. ‘279, Wauron, A, OC, . — Reactions of Sisrationstien caudatnin to Light. Jott: Anita : Behavio: : @ (5); 885-340: Sept-Oct, ‘916. - : dag ‘ 280. “Wauron, A. C. —‘the: * Refractive Body pind. ‘the: “Maitichionasia* of Ascaris = canis Wormers Proa,’ Amer. Acad. Bath and Sel, SH (5) : 268-266, 2'pls. Oct. : . 1946. ‘ 7 BBL. Paxkun, ¢ ‘e. H., axp Trrvs,. B GT. acuie: of Metridium: ‘(Actinoloba : : ‘“inarginatas_ Miine. Edwards with Special Reference to ats Neuro-muscular we - | Mechanisin. Jour. Exp. Zabl-, a1 (4) 1438-450, 1pl Nov.,1916; "939. PxrKer, G. H.—The Hector | By eera of Ag i aa Exp. Zoiley a1 (4): . 461-484, |. Nov,5 1916. 5 1 : “288,,, Wazrow, A.C. Gage of the Occurtence of Ascaris tcigtea Scraak in Dog. a Jour. Paraiitol., $ ay? O41, § spt. [Nov.], 1916..° 284. “PARKAR, G. H,—Nervous Tranemise jon in the Actinians. To. Exp: Zobl., , 22 + (1) :87-04, Jan. 1007. 236, Phaser, G. H.— The Movements of the Tentucles “in Koltieca,- Tours, ap Zod, BB eG) 295-110. 987. Bannan, cup by - # @: 8, “Febs, 1917. trifugution, ana Air Currents Téur. Rae eae (1): 71-80, Jan, ‘1917. = Geotropism in Planaria aboiiats, dom: Anim. Behavior, { xP. Pans 22 oe :106-a20, Feb. sy _ “Jour. Exp. Zi ie 292, “Blepmmp,- Bi! sia : “Broe. Nat. . Acad, Sck:, 3. (3) re 202-208. ‘Mac /19lT.: ig “g9s. , Buby, AW. —tuetsbednatad of the Melanophore Reaction of le ome ~The Skin 0 of: tho Cutts anlar nabalonists 85.4 Re-; (as Lae Bi p1017.: i ied 4 preety ~ 4054205 et pate + Panes 6 * ake ae i Parkise,, @ The Bove nh) Zigiils, BH (2): Bi9-#s CONTRIBUTIONS FROM THE ZOOLOGICAL LABORATORY OF THE MUSEUM OF COMPARATIVE ZOOLOGY "ie HARVARD. _ COLLEGE. (Continued.). Ae es 500. Parker, G. H.—The wctivities of Corymorpha Tour. Exp. casa 24 (2): 308 : 381. Nov., 1917. --° |, P 301. Srainczr, C. E.—The iteanee of Casinbiion: in Planarians.. Proc. Nat. Acad. \ Sci., 3 (12): 691-692. . Dec., 1917. ~ “302. , OLMaTED, J. M. D.—The Regeneration of aeiasgsilar Pieces of Planaria maculata.” ; A Study in Polarity. Jour. Exp. Zodl., 25 (1): 157-176. Feb. 1918. ay tee 308. ; Hacut, S. —The Physiology’ of. Ascidia atra Lesueut. I. General Physiology. r 304. a H. Sensory Physiology.~ Four. Exp. Zobl., B5'(1): 229-299. Feb., 1918. - -~ 206. Huont,$8.—~Id. IIL. ae Blood System. - antes Jour.] nysiol., 45 (3): 157-187. Feb., 1018. Se Be oad 306. Bray, A. W. Le The Secnan af the” Mclanophores of Amiurus to Light and Adrenalin. Proc. Nat. Xeaa. Sci., 4 (3) 58-60, Mar., 1918 -~ } 307, Watton, A. C. —The Odgenesis and Har ly Embryology of ‘Ascaris: ‘eanis Werner. ‘ - Jour, Morbh.. 30 | (2) 2527-608. Mar., 1918. ~~ 308. Omsrxp, J. M. D.— — Expériments on “the Nature of the Sense of Smell i in the __ ‘Common Catfish, Amiurus Henson (Lesueur) é Amér.. Jour. Physiol, 46 (5) 1448-458. Aug., 1918. Te a 309. REDFIELD, A. C.—The. Physiology of the-Melanophores ot the Homed Toad, » 5 Phrynosoma.’ Jour. Exp. Zoil., 26 (2): 275-333. July, 1918. - a ee = 810, Buooxs, BE. S.— Reactions of Frogs to. Heat and Colds, A x. Jour, Physiol es * 46 (5) 1493-501. Aug.,1918.. ee : 811. Cons, P. H.— Autonomous Responses of the” Labial- Palps of Anodonta. Proes. : "Nat. AGads Set, @ (8) 1284-285. Aug. 1018. fe 312. Inwin,M.—The Nature of Sensory. Stimulation by Sate, Amer. a ons Physiol : _ ae (2): 265-277. Nov., 1918. ie 313. Parker, G. H. —Tiie, Rate. of Transmission in nthe Nerve Net of the Coclentenates. A Sours Gen. Physiol. vy (2)+231+4 +236. ~ Nov.; 1918. et aM, Brower, Awe ‘I. —The Effect of Ady enin onthe’ Pigment eaten in tive “Melano- -phiores of the. Skin and i in the Pigment Cells of the Retina. of the, Frog. Te our. Exp. Zobl., 2% (8): 391-396.. Jan,,1919. ~~ ieee ~ B18. Jorpan, H.— Coneerning. Reissnei 3 Fiber id Teleosts: Pex Comp, Neurol., oo 0 5 BO (2): 217-227, lpi.’ Feb., 1919... 316. PARKER, G. H. aan. - Orypuization ‘of Renilla. Jour. Exp. Zo'l., ee a 499 a8 507. Feb., 1919, * a 817, Honr, H.R. ai Trail fength, cm. 21.9 2.95 Ay. Fig. 3 Record of bee no. 123 in non-directive light. angular deflections will give a result equivalent to the amount. of continuous turning required to carry the animal from the start- ing point to the end of its course. Thus, in figure 3, the direction of locomotion at a makes with the direction at e an angle of 23 x 360° — § x 360° or 24 x 360°. Knowing the distance traveled in centimeters and the amount of turning in degrees, the average degrees turned per centimeter is easily computed. Denoting this average deflection, as I shall call it, by D, we have for the trail in figure 3, PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE "357 23 x 360° D = 36.05em. x6 = + 5.01°/ cm. It is to be emphasized that the value + 5.01°/cm., does not sig- nify that the animal turned only toward the functional eye. It merely shows that the algebraic sum of all its deflections aver- ages 5.01°/cm. toward the functional eye. Animal Apis medlifine ; Experiment No. |3 No. of Animal 5 Date Yo, a Time{ 4 50 Am,” @so wc KX bil sag \ aN . x Eye black Lolt << Light 457m NY No. dx. loops a % b&rs_ No. sn. loops a ¥ b ¥ Trail length,cm. a jaa 611 122 12.25 We Wd Ae Fig. 4 Two trails of bee no. 135 in non-directive light. A record is shown in figure 4 which represents two trials taken in rapid succession. This was necessitated by the animal’s en- countering the side wall of the light chamber so quickly that the first trial was shorter than usual. The deflections in these trails are estimated as previously described. It will be noted, however, that in the trail marked a, the angular deflection between m and 7 does not amount to quite 2 of a sinistral loop, although it is so counted. In such instances the angle was always estimated THE JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL ZOOLOGY, VOL. 29, NO. 3 358 DWIGHT E. MINNICH to the nearer } of a circumference, no attempt being made to discriminate differences of less than 45°. In this record it is desirable to combine both trails into a single computation. Pro- ceeding as before, (+$+%3—2— 4) 360° (12.25 + 7.1 cm.) 6 D= = 1.16°/cm. The negative sign of the average deflection here obviously indi- cates that the bee turned more toward the covered eye than toward the functional eye in these trials. In the course of experimentation, records of normal bees were also made in non-directive light. Since in such individuals neither eye was blackened, the positive sign was arbitrarily applied to the direction of greater angular deflection in each set of trials. Other- wise the computations for normal bees were performed in the same manner as those for bees with one eye blackened. These various examples will illustrate the method employed in all quantitative determinations. Upon the results thus obtained the chief conclusions of the present paper are based. IV. MATERIAL 1. General care of animals The bees used in all quantitative experiments were thoroughly active workers taken from the flowers of a near-by garden, and were, for the most part, individuals from a single large hive situ- ated there. The animals were trapped by simply inverting a long glass tumbler over the flower, and then transferring them to a small screen fly-trap. In some experiments, however, which were performed too late in the fall to obtain bees in this way, animals were used from a single comb of workers confined in an observation hive. The exit of the hive was kept securely screened, for such a colony quickly disintegrates if its members are per- mitted to leave the hive freely. Bees kept in this way remained in reasonably good condition, for at least a month. Bees destined to undergo experimentation were first subjected to having their wings clipped, an operation easily executed when PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 359 the animals were feeding. Each wingless individual was then confined in a small cylindrical cage of screen wire, the bottom of which was formed by a layer of tissue-paper over cotton to pre- vent injury in case of falling. In the same cage were also placed two friendly winged workers to counteract any possible effects of isolation. The cages of bees were kept in a darkened box when not directly under experimentation, since the influence of light often caused the animals to maintain a restless activity which appeared, in some cases, to shorten life considerably. In the dark, however, they usually remained more quiet. Each cage was supplied with water by a small wad of saturated cotton placed on its top. Small quantities of honey were also supplied on short wooden sticks stuck to the side of the cage. Early in the morning, at noon, and in the evening the cages were cleaned by removing excess honey, etc., and fresh honey and water were provided. Such operations were carried out at least a half-hour before any trials were made on the animals. The temperature of the laboratory in most of the experiments was kept above 20° to 21°C. This was found to be an important consideration, since at lower temperatures bees became torpid and inactive. In collecting the animals even, an attempt was made to take them on warm, sunny days which had, in general, been preceded by warm weather. It was found that bees brought in after a brief period of cold, wet weather were apt to be either unresponsive or extremely variable in their behavior. 2. Blackening the eye Any technic for blackening the eye of a wingless bee requires, of course, the use of an anaesthetic. In the present.experiments ether was used exclusively. Care was taken to administer it rather slowly and in minimal doses. When completely anaes- thetized, the bee was placed on one side, on a small cork pinning board. Here it was fastened down securely by the use of insect pins, with which the thorax, abdomen, and legs were securely ‘braced against the cork. The blackening was then applied to the eye, the entire surface being covered with as thick a coat as pos- 360 DWIGHT E. MINNICH sible. Two kinds of blackening material were used, viz., lamp- black in shellac and a dead black paint known commercially as ‘Jap-a-lac.’ The latter proved the more satisfactory and was used throughout the majority of experiments. Although bees under ether often began to recover in five to ten minutes, they were not removed from the pinning board for twenty to twenty-five minutes, when the covering of the eye was well hardened. Recovery from anaesthesia was usually com- plete in an hour and often much less. Asa rule, however, opera- tions were carried out in the evening, and the bees were not sub- jected to further experiment until the following day. Ample time was thus allowed for the animals to recover as much as possible from the effects of the operation. V. BEHAVIOR OF NORMAL BEES 1. Kinetic effect of light The remarkable sensitivity of the honey-bee to photic stimu- lation must have long been patent to students of its behavior. Bethe (’98, p. 83) says, ‘‘Das Licht ist bei diesen Tagthieren [bees, flies, etc.] der auslésende Reiz zum Fliegen; in einer dunklen ‘Schachtel fliegt keine Biene auf, auch nicht, wenn man sie reizt. Das Licht gibt die Regulirung beim Fluge ab.’’ This observa- tion was repeatedly confirmed in the present experiments. When collecting bees from flowers, fifteen to twenty individuals were confined in a single cage, which was then placed in a closed box. Although at the height of activity when captured, a few minutes in the darkness of the box seldom failed to reduce these animals to a state of quiescence. If a little light was admitted to the box, however, by even partially removing the lid, there was a sudden resumption of activity. Precisely the same behavior was exhibited by wingless bees. If confined in a dark box, they were, as a rule, reduced to com- parative inactivity. A brief exposure to light, however, was usually sufficient to excite vigorous locomotion, and continued exposure not infrequently resulted in the maintenance of an intense activity for extended intervals of time. PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 361 Individuals which had been subjected to operations of remov- ing the wings and blackening the eye frequently responded some- what more slowly to this photic activation than did normal bees. In the former, locomotion was preceded by a more or less prolonged sequence of cleaning operations. The proboscis was extended and stroked with the fore legs. The eyes, parti¢ularly the covered one, were the objects of repeated and vigorous scrap- ings, responses no doubt largely attributable to the irritation of the blackening material. The abdomen was meanwhile bent from side to side, while the middle or hind legs were rubbed to- gether, or the hind legs assiduously stroked the dorsum of the abdomen. These movements became more and more intense until at length they culminated in active creeping. Light, then, exerts a strong activating or kinetic influence upon the honey-bee, while darkness has the opposite effect. Es- sentially similar phenomena have been reported by Loeb (’90) for the plant louse, Carpenter (05) for the pomace fly, and Turner (’12) for the mason wasp. Stockard (’08) has reported the case of Aplopus, the ‘walking-stick,’ which also falls into this category of behavior. In Aplopus, however, light inhibits ac- tivity, while darkness induces it. Hence the ‘walking-stick’ is nocturnal, whereas the plant louse, the pomace fly, the mason wasp, and the hive bee are diurnal. In diurnal animals this response is apparently due to the con- tinued action of light rather than a sudden change in it. Thus, while many bees respond almost, if not quite, at once to the presence of light, others may respond only after some minutes of exposure. According to Turner (12, p. 360), the same is true of the mason wasp. 2. Directive light Not only does light induce locomotion in the honey-bee, but directive light regulates the course of locomotion. Bees brought into the laboratory direct from their. foraging activities out of doors seldom failed to exhibit a most striking phototropism. Such insects when liberated in the laboratory flew almost im- mediately to the nearest window, where they remained fluttering 362 DWIGHT E. MINNICH against the glass. Or, if escaping in a darkened room, they not infrequently flew directly into the flame of the nearest gas jet. Observations of this sort were long ago reported by Lubbock (82, pp. 278, 279, 284). A few years later, Graber (’84) demon- strated the same thing experimentally by confining forty to sixty bees in a small box, one half of which was illuminated by direct sunlight, the other half being shaded, with the result that the majority of the bees soon collected in the illuminated end. More recently, Hess (’13 a, 713 b, ’17) has repeated this and a variety of other experiments. As a result of these he has been able to show that in the presence of several sources of photic stimulation, which differ in color and intensity, bees always orient toward the one which to a totally color-blind person appears brightest. The positive phototropism of the honey-bee is thus demonstrable in a variety of ways. In the experiments just cited, winged bees were used exclu- sively. My own experiments, on the contrary, were confined en- tirely to workers from which the wings had been clipped. Such bees when creeping in the directive light area exhibited an orien- tation which was striking in three respects, viz., its rapidity, its precision, and its constancy. An individual to be tested was removed from the dark box and exposed to light for a few minutes until it was thoroughly active. It was then allowed to creep from its screen cage to a small, rectangular piece of black paper, and on this it was transferred to the edge of the directive light area. An effort was made to start the animal creeping at a right angle to the direction of the light rays by turning the paper just before it crept off. The rapidity of orientation was so great, however, that the intervening centi- meter or so was frequently sufficient to allow the animal to reorient perfectly. Since the velocity at which bees creep averages 3 to 6 cm. per second, orientation in these cases occurred in considera- bly less than one second. I have also tried leading a bee by moving the light, now in this direction, now in that, with varying degrees of curvature. Always the animal followed, orienting rap- idly to even slight movements of the lamp. PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 363 The precision with which orientation was maintained was no less conspicuous. Once oriented, the animal generally moved in a nearly straight line toward the source of light. In figure 5 are shown two records of each of six bees in the directive light area. Of a large number of animals tested in the course of ex- perimentation, considerably over 25 per cent maintained their orientation as precisely as did bee no. 66. The deviations of ININ/\ AKA Fig. 5 Two trails of each of six normal bees in directive light. In this, as in subsequent figures of records in directive light, the clear circle represents the light source, and the straight lines from it, the direction of the rays. most of the animals would, moreover, easily fall within the lati- tude of that exhibited by bees nos. 66, 33, and 23. Results sim- ilar to those shown for animals nos. 110 and 21 were, on the con- trary, less frequently encountered, while trails such as those of bee no. 36 were seldom or never found among normal, healthy bees. The response to directive light is very constant in the bee. The oncoming of death itself seems often to intensify rather than to weaken this phase of its behavior. Bees occasionally escaped 364 DWIGHT E. MINNICH in the laboratory. Such individuals rarely survived the lack of food for more than a day or so. Yet it was not an infrequent occurrence to observe one of these starved animals, so weak that it was barely able to creep, slowly emerging from a hidden corner in a final struggle toward the light. Nevertheless, bees were discovered which in a few instances failed to exhibit the usual positive reaction to directive light. Suchicases, however, are not to be construed as a total absence ? an SL Fig. 6 Three successive records of a normal bee in directive light, showing failure to orient in two cases. of phototropism, but rather its momentary suppression by other factors of behavior. This is well illustrated by the following example. Seven cages of bees were prepared from the stock in the observation hive, Oct. 30, at 2:45 p.m. When tested about an hour later in the directive light area, six of the seven animals exhibited the usual positive response. One animal, however, gave the records reproduced in figure 6. This bee when given its first trial at 4:06 (fig. 6, 1) did not ori- ent toward the light source. Instead it pursued a devious course PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 365 looping now to the right, now to the left, and finally turning al- most directly away from the light. In a second trial at 4:14, it exhibited a somewhat similar response (fig. 6, 2). One minute later, the animal was subjected to still a third trial, being started on this occasion some 30 cm. nearer the light. This time it ori- ented and moved in a fairly direct course toward the source of illumination (fig. 6, 3). What the temporary, inhibiting factors were which produced these very atypical responses could not be ascertained. In all other respects this bee was quite indistin- guishable from the other individuals in the experiment. This example, however, shows that even the constant response of the bee to directive illumination is not free from abrupt and appar- ently inexplicable departures. 8. Non-directive light The behavior of bees in non-directive light is no less charac- teristic than that in directive illumination. Since all quantita- tive experiments on circus movements were conducted in non- directive light, an intimate acquaintance with the behavior of normal animals under the same conditions was necessary. Every bee was, therefore, subjected to several trials in non-directive light before having one eye blackened. It was a matter of continual observation that a bee creeping in the directive light area ceased to move in a straight course upon reaching the area near and immediately beneath the lamp. Here, where the illumination was essentially non-directive, the animal deflected from its former, precise path and began to loop in a constant or varying direction (fig. 1). In other words, the bee was trapped; for directly it crept away sufficiently for the light to become directive again, it was forced to turn back. Thus the animal continued to creep round and round in a limited area, occasionally rearing on its hind legs in an abortive attempt at flight, or finally ceasing locomotion to begin cleaning operations. In the non-directive light apparatus (fig. 2), the same tend- ency. to loop was manifested, only on a much more extensive scale. Here the bee seldom crept in a.straight line for any great 366 DWIGHT E. MINNICH distance. Each animal was subjected to two sets of trials, an hour or so apart. Usually a single trial only constituted a set. In case the bee quickly encountered the side wall of the light chamber, however, or exhibited unusual variability in its be- havior, additional trials were made. The aggregate duration of the trials of each set varied considerably, even in the same animal. Sometimes they were as short as thirty seconds; again, as long as two minutes. The average was in the neighborhood of thirty to sixty seconds. Preliminary to each trial, the bee was exposed to light until aroused to active creeping. The illumination used throughout in experimenting with normal bees was 957 mc.5 The average deflection to the right or to the left has been computed for each set of records thus obtained, and the results of these computations presented in table 2 (appendix), columns B and C. On the basis of these data, the fifty-two bees experi- mented upon may be classified into three groups: 1. Bees whose average deflection in both sets of trials was over 2°/cem. and in the same direction. 2. Bees whose average deflection in both sets of trials was small. 3. Bees whose average deflection in the two sets of trials varied widely, either in magnitude or direction, or in both. The first class is composed of animals which exhibited a more or less pronounced tendency to turn in a constant direction (right or left). These animals, 29 in number, comprised 56 per cent of the total 52 bees. Fourteen of these were chiefly right-handed in their turns; 15, left-handed. In 14 of the 29 bees, or 27 per cent of the total number, the average deflections exceeded 4° /em., while in 6 individuals, or 12 per cent, it rose to over 8°/em. Sim- ilar right and left-handed tendencies of locomotion in non-direct- ive light have been reported by Walter (’07) for planarians and by Patten (14) for the blowfly larva. A typical example of this behavior in bees is illustrated in figure 7, bee no. 101. In its first trial (fig. 7, 101, a), this animal showed an average deflection of 7.11°/cm. to the left, and in the second trial (fig. 7, 101, b), a sim- ilar deflection of 6.10°/em. Since these records were made nearly an hour apart the left-handed tendency was not the result of a PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 367 10] J \ Left a 36 ig Left — Right Left - Right 1 \ a Sy Left Right »~ b a Fig. 7 Records of normal bees in non-directive light. In this, as in subse- quent figures of records in non-directive light, a solid circle is used to indicate the center of the floor of the non-directive light apparatus. a, records of the first set of trials; b, records of the second set of trials. Bee no. 101 deflected constantly toward the left. Bee no. 36 varied its deflection in the course of single trials. 368 DWIGHT E. MINNICH brief, temporary condition, but was probably a more or less permanent feature of this animal’s behavior. The second class of animals includes those whose average de- flections were small in both directions. The results obtained here are attributable to either of two causes: a. The bee varied its turning from right to left, so that on an average, one tendency nearly or quite balanced the other. b. The bee exhibited little or no tendency to turn either to the right or to the left. An example of the first type is seen in the records of bee no. 36, figure 7. In its first set of trials (a, 1, 2) this animal turned sometimes to the left, sometimes to the right, so that the result- ant average deflection was but 0.79°/cm. to the left. Similarly in the second set of trails (6, 1, 2), the average deflection amounted to only 1.94°/em. to the right. The second type of this class is illustrated by bee no. 134, figure 8. This animal showed no pro- nounced tendency to turn either to the right or left. The aver- age deflection for each set of records was, therefore, small, being only 1.52°/cem. to the left for the first set (a, 1, 2, 3) and 1.22°/cm. to the left for the second set (6, 1, 2, 3, 4). In the third class of bees are to be found those which, although they exhibited fairly uniform behavior in a single set of trials, varied widely in different sets. For example, bee no. 82, in its first record (fig. 8, 82, a) showed a pronounced deflection which averaged 5.81°/cm. to the left. In its second set of trials (fig. 8, 82, 6, 1, 2, 3, 4), on the contrary, it showed little tendency to turn, and the average deflection was but 0.65°/cem. to the left. An even more striking case of variation, however, was afforded by bee no. 63. In a single record of fifty-three seconds’ duration (fig. 8, 63, a) this animal deflected, on the average, 5.58°/em. to the left. Approximately two hours later, in a record of sixty seconds’ duration (fig. 8, 63, b), the same animal exhibited an even greater average deflection in the opposite direction, viz., 7.50°/em. to the right. The range of variation presented by. these two records is no less than 13.08°/em. In a uniform, non-directive light field, therefore, many bees exhibit a fairly constant tendency to turn toward a given side, PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 369 ah S a EAN / Left 134 a Left * a b Fig. 8 Records of normal bees in non-directive light. a, records of the first set of trials; b, records of the second set of trials. Bee no. 134 exhibited little tendency to deflect in either direction. Bees nos. 82 and 63 varied widely in their average deflections in the two sets of trials. 370 DWIGHT £. MINNICH others display little or no such tendency, while still others vary widely in their deflections from time to time. Since the animal moves in a uniform environment, the conspicuous asymmetry of response so frequently noted must be attributed to internal factors. Such factors are, for the most part, probably quite in- dependent of light. A more detailed discussion of these will be presented in a subsequent section of this paper. 4. Total darkness If internal factors are responsible for the asymmetric responses of bees in non-directive illumination, a similar behavior should be exhibited in the total absence of photic stimulation. Such was indeed the case. Animals creeping on smoked paper, in total darkness, showed the same conspicuous tendencies to loop and turn as did animals in non-directive light (fig. 16). The data here referred to were taken in connection with experiments conducted for a different purpose. They are, therefore, not sufficiently extensive to establish more than a similarity to the behavior exhibited in non-directive light. Responses essentially like those of bees in total darkness have also been described by Pouchet (’72) for the larvae of Lucilia caesar, Davenport (’97) for the amoeba, and Frandsen (’01) for the garden slug Limax. Frandsen’s observations in particular bear a striking resemblance to those which I have just described for the honey-bee in non-directive light. Thus he found that while most of his animals looped in a fairly constant fashion to the right or left, a few were extremely variable, while still a few others moved in rather straight courses. The responses of creep- ing bees in the total absence of photic stimulation are, therefore, very similar to those observed for other animals under the same conditions. 6. Summary In the preceding pages certain responses of normal bees have been described in considerable detail, but only as a prerequisite to an adequate understanding of the behavior of the animals PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 371 when one eye is blackened. The features of behavior which are important in this connection may be summarized as follows: 1. In the honey-bee, light tends to induce activity; darkness, to inhibit it. This response is dependent upon the continuous action of photic stimulation. 2. Isolated worker bees in an active condition exhibit strong " positive phototropism when flying or creeping. Temporary sup- pressions of this response may occur, however. 3. Normal bees when creeping in non-directive light usually exhibit pronounced asymmetrical responses of constant or vari- able index. Since essentially the same responses occur in total darkness they are not fundamentally dependent upon photic stimulation. They are probably, therefore, conditioned largely by internal factors. VI. BEHAVIOR OF BEES WITH ONE EYE BLACKENED 1. Directive light The previous investigations of circus movements have pointed unmistakably to the generality of these responses among photo- tropic arthropods. Positive animals with one eye covered tend to circle toward the functional eye; negative animals, under the same conditions, tend to circle away from the functional eye. The honey-bee exhibits a striking positive phototropism. When one eye is blackened, therefore, we should expect the bee to circle toward the remaining functional eye. Such is indeed the case, as Axenfeld (’99, p. 374) has previously shown. In my own experiments, bees thus operated upon were no longer able to creep in a straight course toward a source of il- lumination. Instead, their progress thither was marked by re- peated loops. If the right eye was blackened, the bee looped to the left; if the left eye was blackened, it looped to the right. Moreover, it was possible by blackening one eye, then removing the black and blacking the other eye, to cause a single individual to perform circus movements first in one direction, then in the opposite direction. 372 DWIGHT E. MINNICH The above experiment was carried out on five bees. In gen- eral, all of these animals looped more or less markedly toward the functional eye as they crept toward the light source. This tend- ency, moreover, was not confined to the period immediately subsequent to the operation of blackening the eye, as the experi- ment clearly demonstrates. The first records of these bees with their left eyes blackened were taken in the evening between 6 and 7 p.m. No further tests were made until the following day at 11:30 am. Yet the behavior at the end of this seventeen- hour period was practically the same as it had been before. One bee, it is true, showed considerable improvement. In the other four animals, however, the two sets of records were indistinguish- able. In the absence of experience, therefore, the performance of circus movements remains a permanent feature of behavior. Of the five bees tested, the most pronounced and uniform exhi- bition of circus movements was displayed by bee no. 5. Its’ records are almost diagrammatic in their close approximation to the theoretical expectation. Records of this animal are repro- duced in figure 9. Not all of these animals, however, yielded such striking results. Some individuals were found which manifested little or no tend- ency to deviate toward the functional eye, except in the area immediately beneath the lamp, where the illumination was es- sentially non-directive. Thus, bee no. 4, with its right eye black- ened, circled toward the left in the usual manner. But a few hours later, when the black had been removed from the right eye and the left eye painted over, it exhibited little or no tend- ency to circle toward the right (fig. 10, B). The explanation at once suggests itself, that in such cases the eye was imperfectly covered, and hence not absolutely free from stimulation. This may be correct. As will be shown later, however, there are also a variety of other circumstances which might account for such behavior. The tendency to circle toward the blackened eye was not fre- quently encountered in the reactions of bees to directive light. No instance of it occurred in the experiment described above, al- though it was occasionally met with in other experiments. A PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 373 / | @ It Normal Right Eye Black. Left Eye Black Normal Left Eye Black \\ Rt yy OW Fig. 9 Consecutive records of a bee in directive light, showing the effect of blackening first one eye, then the other. THE JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL ZOGLOGY, VOL. 29, NO. 3 374 DWIGHT E. MINNICH single record of this kind is shown in figure 10, A. This was ob- tained from an animal which subsequently became wholly unre- active. Its aberrant tendencies may, therefore, have been due to an abnormal condition. In any case it is significant that, al- though the bee looped toward the covered eye, yet it progressed toward the light source. Consequently, this was not a case of reversal of phototropism. Instances somewhat similar to the one last mentioned have been described by Dolley (’16, p. 373) for the butterfly Vanessa. i i" ir | \U [2 Fig. 10 A. Record of a bee in directive light, showing loops toward the blackened eye. B. Two records of a bee in directive light which showed no deflection, although the left eye was blackened. Although positive to light, this insect with one eye blackened occasionally turned toward the covered eye instead of toward the functional eye. Possibly results of this sort are to be attributed to the effect of contact stimulus afforded by the covering of the eye, as indicated by Dolley (’16, pp. 394-399). This will be dis- cussed more fully in a subsequent portion of the paper. 2. Non-directive light a. Amount of turning. In non-directive light, bees with one eye blackened tended, in general, to turn more or less pronouncedly toward the functional eye. As was to be expected, the course PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 375 taken by the animals under these conditions assumed no specific direction. They either continued to circle in a fairly limited area or proceeded in a looping fashion in any direction whatever. The variability of this response, moreover, was much greater than in the case of the response to directive light. Thus, a num- ber of animals circled almost continuously toward the covered eye in non-directive light, while still others varied, circling some- times toward the covered eye, sometimes toward the functional eye. This was doubtless true for much the same reasons that normal bees also exhibited a greater variability of response in non-directive light. Circus movements attendant upon the elimination of one pho- toreceptor undoubtedly represent the orienting process of an asymmetric animal. The specific photic stimulus, therefore, which produces these reactions must be identical with that which effects orientation in the normal animal. Whatever the nature of this stimulus be, moreover, it is afforded by both directive and non-directive light, since circus movements occur in either ‘situation. What is the nature of this orienting stimulus? Per- haps the best method of demonstrating the dependence of a par- ticular response upon a certain stimulus is to show that the in- tensity of the response varies with the intensity of the stimulus in question. It seemed possible to attack this problem, therefore, through a study of the relationship existent between the amount of turning displayed by an animal with one eye blackened and the intensity of the illumination to which it was subjected. Non-directive illumination was chosen in preference to direc- tive illumination because of the simpler experimental conditions which the former affords. In directive light, every movement of the entire animal is accompanied by more or less complicated changes not only in the intensity of the stimulation received, but also in the area of the eye stimulated. Asan animal with one eye covered moves toward a light source, the stimulation of the functional eye steadily increases. As it loops toward this eye, however, this steadily increasing stimulus is subjected to rapid and transitory fluctuations. When the animal begins to loop, the functional eye is first turned away from the light, resulting 376 DWIGHT E. MINNICH in a rapid decrease of photic stimulation. As the loop is com- pleted, the photoreceptor in turn experiences an increase in stimulation. In non-directive illumination such as was employed in the present experiments, however, these complications are largely avoided. Photic stimulation here is maintained at a fairly uniform and constant intensity over the entire surface of the compound eye. Two slightly different types of experiment were performed. The procedure in the first type was as follows. Bees were col- lected from flowers in the morning between 8 and 10 o’clock, brought into the laboratory and prepared for experimentation. One and two hours later, respectively, they weré given single trials in the directive light area. On a basis of these records, individuals of abnormal tendencies were discarded, and those evinc- ing the greatest accuracy of orientation were selected. An hour or so later, the selected bees were tested in non- directive light of 957 mc. Two sets of records, about one hour apart, were made of each animal. Each set was composed of one to several records, the aggregate duration of which, in gen- eral, was between thirty and sixty seconds. An examination of the records showed clearly whether the individual was normally right-handed, left-handed, or variable in its deflection in non- directive light. These results determined which eye should be blackened. If, for example, a bee normally circled to the right, the right eye was covered. Whatever influence was exerted by photic stimulation, therefore, would tend to force the animal toward the left. In this manner, respohses which might other- wise have been mistakenly attributed to photic stimulation were to some extent eliminated. The operations of blackening the eye were carried out in the late afternoon of the first day of experimentation, in accordance with the technic previously described. On the following morn- ing, before resuming experimentation, it was not infrequently necessary to discard a few additional animals either because of extreme weakness or occasionally death as a result of the operation. The majority of bees usually appeared quite normal, however, and were subjected to several series of trials in non-directive PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 377 light. Throughout a single series of consecutive trials, or, as I shall call it, a determination, one intensity of light only was employed. But in the total number of determinations the more intense illumination of 957 me. and the less intense of 24 mc. were used an equal number of times. The animal to be tested was first removed from the dark box and exposed from half a minute to several minutes in the intensity of light in which it was to be tried. This was usually sufficient to activate the animal thor- oughly, and several records were then made in the non-directive light chamber. In case the bee failed to respond to photic acti- vation, recourse was had to mechanical stimulation. The cage was tapped or even shaken fairly vigorously until locomotion was induced. This procedure seldom failed to elicit activity. When it did fail, it was usually necessary to discard the animal altogether. The number and duration of the records comprising a single series or determination varied widely even in the same animal. If the bee quickly encountered the side wall of the light chamber, records were short, and a number had to be taken. If, on the contrary, the animal kept well toward the center of the floor of the apparatus, one or two records were quite sufficient. In cases of great variability of response or unusual departures from the general, expected behavior, additional trials were made, on the assumption that a greater number would more accurately ex- press the average tendency of the animal. Single trials seldom exceeded thirty seconds, and were often much shorter. Occa- sionally, however, records of forty-five seconds, sixty seconds, or even slightly greater durations were taken. The aggregate duration of the trials comprising a single determination, for one intensity of light, was usually in the neighborhood of thirty or sixty or ninety seconds. The adoption of any more uniform period for all animals, at all times, was quite impossible. Upon completion of a series of trials in one intensity of light, the bee was returned to the dark box. Here it was allowed to remain for a period of about fifteen minutes to one hour. In the earlier experiments the longer period was practiced ; in subsequent experiments, the shorter. After this period in the dark, the ani- 378 DWIGHT E. MINNICH mal was subjected to a second set of trials of the same aggregate duration as the first, but in the other of the two light intensities. The order in which the two intensities of illumination were em- ployed was varied from time to time. Sometimes the first de- termination was made in the more intense light; the second, in the less intense. Sometimes the reverse order was observed. A single series of records in one intensity of non-directive light together with the corresponding series in the other intensity con- stitute what I shall term a pair of determinations. The protocol of such a pair of determinations on bee no. 42 is given in table 3. Four or five pairs of determinations were usually made on each in- dividual of an experiment in the course of a day, beginning TABLE 3 DETERMINATION FOR NON-DIRECTIVE LIGHT DETERMINATION FOR NON-DIRECTIVE LIGHT or 24 Mc. , oF 957 Mc. N i , winhiee of Hour of record D uration of ene of Hour of record che of seconds seconds 4 1:47 p.m 30 1 1:32 p.m. 31 5 1:473 p.m. 30 2 1:322 p.m. 30 6 1:48 p.m -30 3 1:334 p.m. 30 id Boo) | ee ee 90 91 between 8 and 9 o’clock in the morning and concluding between 4 and 5 in the afternoon. The bees often seemed to become slug- gish in the late afternoon. Whether this was due to fatigue or a natural rhythm of activity from day to night, I am unable to say. This phenomenon, however, led me to abandon any attempt to continue experimentation much after 5 o’clock. On the third and concluding day of the experiment, the scheme of the second day was again carried out as far as possible. Bees usually survived the first two days of experimentation, and in case they did not, the data on them were discarded. A nianbes of individuals, however, failed to survive in fit condition for the trials of the third day, and still others had to be discarded in the course of the day, although in both cases the results were counted. PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 379 Some of the more vigorous animals survived not only a third day of experimentation, but lived on for three or four days, and in a few instances even longer. Although no further trials were made with such bees, they were kept and, as far as possible, records of their subsequent longevity taken. Having described the first type of non-directive light experi- ment in considerable detail, the second type may be described very briefly. It differed from the first only in the method of making pairs of determinations. In this case, the two determi- nations of a pair were made during the same period of time, TABLE 4 DETERMINATION FOR NON-DIRECTIVE LIGHT DETERMINATION FOR NON-DIRECTIVE LIGHT OF 24 Mc. oF 957 mc. Number of Duration of Number of Durati f record Hour of record record record Hour of record euoed seconds seconds 1 1:41 p.m. 30 2 1:43 p.m. il 3 1:48 p.m. 44 4 1:51 p.m. 23 5 1:53 p.m. 40 6 1:56 p.m. 30 7 1:59 p.m. 30 TotalStecscssa4 ayers <0 oe 104 104 instead of an appreciable interval apart. The bee was first tested in one intensity of light, then within a minute or so in the other intensity, then again in the first, and so on until a series of one to five records had been completed for each intensity. Care was exercised, however, even with this rapid alternation of intensi- ties, always to expose the animal for thirty to sixty seconds in a given intensity before subjecting it to a trialin the same. The following protocol from bee no. 83 (table 4) will illustrate this method of making determinations. In both types of experiment, there were obtained for each bee a number of pairs of determinations, usually four to ten, depend- ing upon the longevity of the individual. The records of each 380 DWIGHT E. MINNICH determination have been computed collectively in the manner already described. Single values have thus been derived which express the average deflection, or tendency to turn, exhibited by the animal in each determination. When the turning was chiefly toward the blackened eye, the sign of these values is negative; when chiefly toward the functional eye, it is positive. If, now, the value of each determination in 24 me. light be subtracted from the corresponding one in 957 me. light, differences will be obtained which should answer conclusively the question of rela- tionship between the amount of turning and the intensity of photic stimulation. The differences obtained in the manner just described I shall designate as d._ A given value of d may be negative or positive. If it be negative, it signifies one of the two following possibilities: 1. The bee turned more toward the blackened eye in an illumina- tion of 957 me. than it did in one of 24 me. For example, in the second pair of determinations on bee no. 32 (table 2), 957 me. 24 me. —13.28°/em. — (—9.22°/em.) = —4.06°/em. 2. The bee turned less toward the functional eye in an illumi- nation of 957 mc. than it did in one of 24 me. For example in the second pair of determinations on bee no. 23, 957 me. 24 me. +6.44°/em. — (+8.92°/em.) = —2.48°/em. If, however, the value of d be positive, it signifies one of the two following possibilities: 1. The bee turned less toward the blackened eye in an illumina- tion of 957 mc. than wt did in one of 24 me. For example, in the first pair of determinations on bee no. 31, 957 me. 24 me. —3.87°/em. — (—7.14°/em.) = +3.27°/em. 2. The bee turned more toward the functional eye in an illumi- nation of 957 me. than in one of 24 me. PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 381 For example, in the first pair of determinations on bee no. 21, 957 me. 24 mc. +29.41°/em. — (+15.84°/em.) = +13.57°/em. If the great majority of d values are of the first category, viz., negative, we may conclude that the animal experiences a greater impulse to turn toward the functional eye in an illumination of 24 me. than it does in one of 957 me. If equal numbers of nega- tive and positive values occur, there is no relation between the intensities of photic stimulation employed and the amount of turning. If, however, d is generally positive, we may conclude that the tendency to turn toward the functional eye increases if the intensity of photic stimulation is sufficiently increased. Experimentation soon demonstrated that the only satisfactory solution of the problem was to be had through a statistical treat- ment of large numbers of data. Even the more constant animals often varied widely from one pair of determinations to another without any apparent external cause. Therefore, a large num- ber of bees were experimented upon and each individual was sub- jected to many tests, the averages of which were relied upon to indicate the general trend of behavior. In table 2 (appendix) are presented the results obtained from a careful measurement and computation of over two thousand records taken on fifty-two bees. On some individuals as few as sixteen records were taken; on others as many as seventy-four. This difference was due in part to varying longevity of individuals and in part to the fact that more favorable animals were frequently experimented with longer than less favorable ones. The determinations of approxi- mately the first half of the animals were made on the plan of the: first type of experiment, while the remainder were carried out according to the scheme used in the second type. From the figures presented in columns F and G of table 2, it is evident at once that there is a marked preponderance of the positive d values over the negative. The ratio of the two is strikingly shown in the frequency polygon (fig. 11) Since the number of d values varied considerably with the individual, due 382 DWIGHT E. MINNICH to the causes noted above, I have included in the polygon only the first four values for each bee, thus giving equal weight to every animal. Of the total two hundred and seven*® values rep- resented in the polygon, 81.16 per cent are positive, whereas: only 18.84 per cent are negative, a ratio of over 4 to 1. [42 18.84% 81.16% 26 | 26 yt] Degrees per cm. Fig. 11 Frequency polygon of the first four d values of fifty-two bees. The negative values are represented by the shaded areas; th iti sical ; the positive values, by the ®In the case of one bee it was possible to include of only th because of a missing record. See Table 2 (Appendix), hee goes PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 383 The objection might be raised that although the majority of values of d are positive, a number of them are too small to be of any significance. It is true that differences of the order of 1°/cm. -or less might easily be attributed to slight errors in tracing the course of a bee. Errors in recording, however, are as likely to occur in one direction as the other. Such is not the case with these small values. The class of d values betwen 0 and —2°/cm. contains but fifteen, while the class of 0 to +2°/cm. numbers twenty-nine—nearly twice as many. Moreover, the mode of the curve, +2°/cm. to +4°/em., lies well beyond these small values. Differences of this magnitude — are readily detected in the records of bees which circled constantly toward the functional eye, as the pair of determinations in figure 12 demonstrate. Each record shown in the figure was of exactly thirty seconds’ duration. The first two were taken three min- utes apart in 24 me. illumination, while the third and fourth were taken about twenty minutes later in 957 me. illumination, one minute apart. The value of d in this case is +2.98°/em.— about the average modal value. In bees exhibiting considerable variation in their deflections, however, d values, or of the modal class of even greater magni- tude, are not so easily recognized without the accompanying fig- ures. To illustrate this, I have selected a pair of determinations (fig. 13) approximating the mean value of the frequency polygon, which is +4.35°/em. As attested by the data given in connec- tion with the figure, this animal was extremely variable in its direction of turning. All four records were taken in the brief period of eight minutes, nos. 1 and 2 being of twenty-eight sec- onds’ duration each; nos. 3 and 4, of thirty seconds. The value of dis +4.09°/cm.—a fact which does not become apparent until the records are submitted to a careful scrutiny. The data presented in table 2 and figure 11 show clearly that bees with one eye blackened tend to turn more toward the func- tional eye in a non-directive illumination of 957 mc. than they do in one of 24 mec. The validity of this conclusion is confirmed by still another line of evidence. A certain number of animals were found to exhibit a very constant tendency to turn toward 384 DWIGHT E. MINNICH the functional eye, which was always much more pronounced in the intense than in the weak illumination. In other words, these individuals always yielded positive values of d of rather high mag- nitude. Bees nos. 73, 83, 95, 105, and to a lesser extent numer- ous others afforded striking examples of such behavior. These 24 me. \ ; A 4 ; = @ ; Fig. 12 A pair of determinations of bee no. 42, left eye black, non-directive light.“ The records are numbered in the order in which they were taken. 957 me, 24 me. Light 9657 me, Light Number +Degrees —Degrees ee ais +Deerees — Degrees vecord turned turned record turned turned 1 900 0 3 1440 0 2 945 0 4 1485 0 Average deflection, +9.49° /em. Average deflection, +12.47°/cm. d = +2.98°/cm. animals were all thoroughly vigorous individuals, surviving not only the three days of experimentation, but living on for at least two days thereafter. Two of these bees, in fact, survived no less than four days after the conclusion of the experiment. In figures 14 and 15 are shown pairs of determinations from two of these animals. The eight records of bee no. 73 (fig. 14) were taken in the course of 28; minutes, while the six records of PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 385 24 mc. 1 — 2 957 me. ae os . “— e 4 = / ! 3 Fig. 13 A pair of determinations of bee no. 123, left eye black, non-directive light. The records are numbered in the order in which they were taken. 24 me. Light 967 mc. Light see +Degrees — Degrees ere Mer +Degrees . —Degrees #enord turned turned tecordl turned turned 2 495 1170 1 630 0 4 270 450 3 225 720 Average deflection, —3.53° /em. Average deflection, +0.56°/cm. d = +4.09° /em. 386 DWIGHT E. MINNICH 24 mc. ie) 5 7 957 mc. @ e a 2 4 6 8 Fig. 14 A pair of determinations of bee no. 73, left eye black, non-directive light. The records are numbered in the order in which they were taken. 24 me. Light 957 me. Light Number +Deegrees — Degrees Buber +Degrees —Degrees Seeard| turned turned record turned turned 1 360 90 2 1485 0 3 45 0 4 990 0 5 2115 0 6 3510 0 7 2070 225 8 4005 0 Average deflection, +6.07°/cm. Average deflection, +13.82° /em. d = +7.75° /em. PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 387 bee no. 105 (fig. 15) required only fifteen minutes. In both figures, each record of the top row is of exactly the same duration as the corresponding one of the lower row, except records 1 and 2 of bee no. 73, which differ by one second. It would be difficult to 24 mc. e) & Se, a mc. Co EF & Fig. 15 A pair of determinations of bee no. 105, right eye black, non-directive light. The records are numbered in the order in which they were taken. 24 me. Light 957 me. Light sales abe +Degrees — Degrees Dineihe -+Degrees — Degrees record turned turned Feoord turned turned 1 1260 45 2 1935 0 3 855 0 4 2430 0 5 1305 90 6 2520 0 Average deflection, +6.74° /cm. Average deflection, +14.83°/cm. “d= +8.09°/em. imagine more conclusive results than those afforded by these two bees. , It might be supposed that animals would be found which would exhibit the opposite of the condition just described. Such, how- ever, was not the case. I failed to find any individuals which continually circled more toward the functional eye in an illumi- 388 DWIGHT E. MINNICH nation of 24 me. than they did in one of 957 mc. Bees presenting a number of negative d values, such as nos. 22, 25, 34, 41, 43, 55, 56, 62, 66, 85, 126, and 135, with one exception, showed an equal or greater number of positive values. The exception noted was bee no. 56. Three of the four pairs of determinations obtained on this animal not only yielded negative differences, but differ- ences of large magnitude as well. Bee no. 56, like a number of other individuals presenting a considerable number of negative d values, varied considerably in its behavior and turned chiefly toward the blackened eye. How far the disturbing factors thus evidenced account for the results is not absolutely certain, since a number of bees of apparently similar tendencies yielded posi- tive values of d. Certainly, however, there are a number of factors, particularly in the type of experiment under considera- tion, which do interfere with the effect of photic stimulation. Some of these serve to intensify the response, while others tend to: counteract or even completely annul it. Without attempting to: minimize the significance of these negative data in the least, I believe some of them, probably all of them, find their explana- tion in such factors. If this be correct, the negative results ob- tained lie well: within the range of variation which might be expected in experimental work of this sort. A more extended discussion of the factors responsible for variability of behavior in the present experiments is presented in the next section of this paper. The evidence in general, therefore, seems to warrant the con- clusion that bees with one eye blackened tend to turn more toward the functional eye in an illumination of 957 me. than in one of 24mc. This tendency may result in the animal’s actually turn- ing more toward the functional eye, or in its turning less toward the covered eye, depending upon the idiosyncrasies of the indi- vidual. In either case, however, with increased photic stimula- tion, there is an increased tendency toward the functional eye. The nature of the stimulus afforded by the apparatus employed was continuous and of almost uniform intensity, and since the ey eae ee vary with the intensity of ) 8 pendent upon it. These con- PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 389 clusions are the exact antitheses of those reached by Dolley (’16) in his work on Vanessa. He says (p. 417): ‘“‘Vanessas with one eye blackened do not move in smaller circles in strong light than they do in weak light, unless it is extremely low. On the con- trary, the evidence seems to indicate that the stronger the light is the larger the circles are. These results also are not in har- mony with those demanded by the ‘continuous action theory.’ ”’ I shall return later to a more complete consideration of the theo- retical significance of the results afforded by the honey-bee. b. Rate of locomotion. Although bees with one eye blackened tend to turn more toward the functional eye in a non-directive light of 957 mc. than they do in one of 24 mc., there is no differ- ence in the rate of locomotion in the two intensities. In table 5 are given the average velocities of thirty-four bees for each of the two light intensities employed. These figures show a con- siderable range of individual variation, from as low as 3.49 cm. per second to as high as 6.77 cm. per second. There is, however, no consistent difference which might be attributed to the effect of light. Eighteen of the animals showed a greater velocity in 957 me. illumination; sixteen, in 24 me. illumination. Unilateral photic stimulation of the intensities employed is, therefore, without effect upon the rate of locomotion. 3. Summary 1. Bees with-one eye blackened usually loop toward the func- tional eye ‘as they creep toward a source of light. Some indi- viduals are encountered however, which display little tendency to loop, and occasionally an animal will be found which loops toward the covered eye. In the absence of experience, the tendency to loop toward the functional eye remains a permanent feature of behavior. 2. In non-directive light, bees with one eye blackened gener- ally circle toward the functional eye, although a number are found which circle more or less toward the covered eye. 3. In a uniform non-directive illumination of 957 mce., the tendency to turn toward the functional eye is greater than it is in a similar illumination of 24 me. THE JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL ZOOLOGY, VOL. 29, No. 3 390 DWIGHT E. MINNICH 4. Since the amount of turning varies directly with the intensity of continuous photic stimulation, the turning is produced by this stimulus. 5. Unilateral photic stimulation of the intensities employed has no effect upon the rate of locomotion. TABLE 5 A B c D NUMBER OF BEE Sica yeas stata SEC, Brey cee ae SEC. aera a 6.10 6.22 +0.12 22 5.10 5.12 +0.02 23 5.02 5.03 +0.01 a 4.95 : 5.18 +0.23 25 4.67 4.53 —0.14 31 3.77 4.28 +0.46 32 4,20 4.74 +0.54 33 5.27 5.19 —0.08 34 4.78 4.99 +0.21 be ed 3.63 +0.14 41 4.45 4.29 —0.16 42 4.76 4.30 —0.46 43 5.59 5.76 40.17 44 4.94 4.90 —0.04 45 4.36 4.99 +0.63 51 4.51 4.40 0.11 52 5.09 5.08 0.01 53 6.77 6.53 0.24 . 54 5.53 5.17 0.36 55 6.09 5.21 , 0.88 56 5.32 5.53 +0.21 62 4.82 459 ¢ wa 58 - 5.80 5.72 ~0.08 5.10 515 Feces 68 5.14 5.34 40.20 72 4.69 4.87 40.18 n ae 5.44 +0.47 7 4.26 3.99 0.97 81 4.41 4.32 0.09 pe 5.25 "5.29 +0.04 88 5.22 4.93 tie - 4.91 4.79 Sie 91 4.48 464. é ae 92 4.49 i +0.16 es +0,02 PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 391 VII. VARIABILITY OF PHOTIC RESPONSE The honey-bee is remarkably constant in the strong positive phototropism which it evinces. The course of individuals creep- ing in directive light is a straight path toward the source. Yet, as has been shown, occasional departures from this behavior do occur. In non-directive light, moreover, the responses of normal bees are frequently extremely variable. The animal may turn markedly toward a given side in one trial, and in the next, turn quite as markedly toward the opposite side. Again, the direc- tion of turning may be completely changed several times in the course of a single trial. It is not surprising, therefore, that bees with one eye blackened also exhibit considerable variability of response in both directive and non-directive illumination. In non-directive light particu- larly, there were a number of cases in which animals turned little or not at all toward the functional eye, while there were still others in which they circled chiefly toward the covered eye. In fact, over 25 per cent of the first four pairs of determinations on the fifty-two bees, when averaged, gave negative values. These departures from the more usual tendency, to turn toward the functional eye, sometimes characterized the entire behavior of an individual; again, they appeared only spasmodically. Thinking that some of the results above mentioned might be attributed to a loss of phototropism, either permanent or extend- ing over a considerable interval of time, I frequently subjected the animals exhibiting them to one or more immediate trials in direc- tive light. This, however, failed to show anything which might be construed as a loss of phototropism. The variations of response noted, therefore, must be referred to external and internal factors which are capable of modifying, in a more or less profound way, the dominating effect of unilateral photic stimulation. Such factors are of two sorts, those which are continuously effec- tive and those which are not continuously effective, but fluctuate from time to time. Both types played so considerable a réle in the experiments previously described, that I have felt they mer- ited the somewhat extended analysis presented in the following, pages. 392 DWIGHT E. MINNICH 1. Continuous factors a. Temperature and humidity. Of the continuously operative factors, none are more important than the general conditions of temperature and humidity. These profoundly affect the activity of bees. MelIndoo (’14, p. 279) says: “Climatic conditions per- ceptibly affect the activity of bees. When it is extremely warm, they are most active and are rarely quiet even for a few seconds. When it is moderately warm, they are less restless, and when rather cool, bees do not move freely.” Again he says: ‘‘ During cool weather their movements are quite sluggish, and when the humidity is high they are much less active and respond to vari- ous odors more slowly than when there is low humidity.” Precisely the same effects were noted in the present experi- ments. So serious did they become on several occasions that the ' experiment had to be abandoned. On cool, damp days bees were apt to be quite unreactive, and prolonged exposure to light often failed to induce locomotion. Considerable mechanical stim- ulation might call forth creeping, but it was of desultory kind and was apparently unaffected by photic stimulation. There can be no doubt, therefore, that general weather conditions consid- erably affect the behavior of bees toward light. Since some experiments were continued under less favorable weather con- ditions, it is quite probable that they account for some of the aberrancies observed. b. After-effects. In making quantitative determinations in non-directive light, trials were frequently made in the two inten- sities. in rapid succession. Although the bee was always exposed for at least thirty seconds to a given intensity before making a test, there still existed the possibility that an after effect of the first intensity might influence the behavior of the animal in the sec- ond. Thus Herms (711, p. 215) has demonstrated an after-effect of photic stimulation in the blow-fly larva, which may manifest itself in the continued orientation of the animal for as much as fifteen to twenty seconds after the cessation of the stimulus. That such is not the case for bees, however, is very clearly dem- -onstrated by the following experiment. PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 393 Normal bees were allowed to creep on a strip of smoked paper toward the 80-c.p. incandescent lamp. When, after creeping a distance of about 60 cm., the bee reached a point 40 cm. from the source of light, the lamp was extinguished. Not only was the current turned off, but a screen was simultaneously placed before the lamp, so that even the slight after-glow of the filaments was eliminated. After ten seconds of total darkness the light was turned on and the bee removed. Two to four records were made of each of eleven animals in this manner. In not one case did the bee fail to lose its orientation within at most a couple of seconds after the extinction of the light. -Often the loss of ori- entation was almost, if not quite, simultaneous with the cessa- tion of the stimulus. The deviation from the former orienta- tion was sometimes marked by a pronounced tendency to turn toward one side, with the result that the animal crept in circles in the dark. At other times the bee merely wandered, turning first in one direction, then another. Typical records of two ani- mals are reproduced in figure 16. There is, therefore, no after- effect of photic stimulation in the honey-bee, and this does not account for any of the irregularities observed. c. Failure to elominate photoreceptor. The difficulties in manipu- lating bees necessitate relying upon a single operation to elimi- nate completely the function of the compound eye. Although in this operation great care was exercised to cover the eye com- pletely with a heavy coat of paint, there isa possibility that in a few cases this was not accomplished. Moreover, from the moment the animal began to recover from the anaesthetic, the covered eye was repeatedly subjected to vigorous scrapings by the front leg of the same side. While the examination of a number of animals after experimentation showed that this seldom resulted in a removal of any of the eye covering, it probably succeeded in doing so in a few cases. Occasionally, also, the varnish cracked somewhat on drying. These unavoidable failures to keep the compound eye entirely free of light, undoubtedly modified, to a greater or less extent, not a few of the results obtained. Beside the failure to cover the eye, there is the possibility that the three ocelli of the honey-bee are concerned in its photic 394 DWIGHT E. MINNICH Fig. 16 Smoked-paper records of two normal bees creeping toward a light source (upward in the figure). As the animals reached the line indicated by the letter P the light was extinguished. Note the extremely rapid loss of orientation, PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 395 behavior. In blackening the compound eye I made no attempt to cover the ocelli, but afterward in examining the specimens used, I found that sometimes all, sometimes only one or two, at other times none of these organs had been covered. If the ocelli do exercise any considerable function, therefore, there was involved here a variable of no small magnitude. It is also possible that in bees other portions of the body, or even the entire integument, may be photosensitive. Photoder- matic sensitivity is not unknown among arthropods. It has been reported by Graber (’84) for the cockroach, by Plateau (’87) for two species of blind myriapods, and by Stockard (’08) for the walking-stick. In order to find out if these several possibilities were affecting results, I conducted several experiments with bees both eyes of which had been carefully covered with a thick coat of ‘Jap-a-lac.’ On the morning after the operation, each bee was placed in a separate cage in non-directive illumination of 957 mc. Although with one or two exceptions the bees were quiet when first exposed to the light, within fifteen minutes all had become thoroughly active, showing clearly that they were not free from the activat- ing effect of the light. ‘At the conclusion of the above test the same bees were individu- ally subjected to trials in the directive light area. Here they looped and turned in a variety of ways, some circling more or less constantly toward a given side, not unlike bees with only one eye blackened. Despite most devious courses, however, they sooner or later managed to work their way to the general region of the source of light. It is quite clear, therefore, that photore- ception had not been entirely abolished, although both com- pound eyes had been covered as carefully as possible. Which of the several explanations advanced accounts for these results, is not certain. I am disposed to believe that the failure to eliminate the compound eyes completely was chiefly, perhaps solely, responsible. However that may be, it is certain that in this, as well as in other experiments, the incomplete suppression of photic stimulation was the source of a large amount of varia- tion in the results obtained. 396 DWIGHT E. MINNICH d. Effect of contact stimulus. The effect of the contact stimu- lus afforded by the blackening material on the eye and the adja- cent parts of the head must also be recognized as a considerable factor in modification of photic behavior. The influence of this stimulus has been clearly demonstrated by Dolley (16, pp. 394—- 397) on Vanessa antiopa. With one eye blackened, this butter- fly, when creeping in total darkness, turned, with few exceptions, continuously toward the blinded eye. The tendency to circle was often quite pronounced, and showed little or no modification from day to day. The effect of contact stimulus on the covered eye was, therefore, antagonistic to that produced by light on the opposite, functional eye. I tried experiments with the honey-bee similar to those car- ried out by Dolley on Vanessa. Bees with one eye blackened were allowed to creep on smoked paper in total darkness. Un- fortunately, the trials were of such duration that the bee recrossed its course many times. This made it quite impossible to decipher the records, and I have not since had an opportunity to repeat the experiments. It is not unlikely, however, that the effect of con- tact stimulus on the eye of the bee is similar to that demonstrated for Vanessa. If such be the case, it probably accounts for much of the circling toward the covered eye, which was evinced by numerous individuals particularly in non-directive light. e. Asymmetry of the animal. As was previously pointed out, normal bees frequently exhibited a marked tendency to turn more or less constantly toward the right or left when creeping in non- directive light. Such tendencies are doubtless due to a lack of perfect symmetry on the part of the animal. The asymmetry may be physiological; it may be anatomical. It may consist of a differential sensitivity of the photoreceptors on the two sides of the body, as Patten (’14, p. 259) has suggested, or it may be due to an inequality of any two symmetrically located elements of the neuromuscular mechanism. Under the influence of directive light, these idiosyncracies are, as a rule, continually corrected. In non-directive illumination or total darkness, however, they at once assert themselves. Since the eye to be blackened was always chosen so that the effect of photic stimulation would be opposite PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 397 to that exerted by natural asymmetry, many failures to turn toward the functional eye are probably thus accounted for. f. Modifiability through experience. The work of Axenfeld, Holmes, Brundin, and Dolley has shown that a number of ar- thropods are able to modify their photic behavior through expe- rience. The same is true of the honey-bee, at least in directive light, as the following experiment shows. Each of a number of normal bees, selected on the basis of the accuracy with which they oriented to directive light, had one eye blackened. On the fol- lowing day those animals which exhibited a more or less pro- nounced tendency to loop toward the functional eye were sub- jected to trials (twenty to twenty-five in number) in the directive light area. Bees which displayed little or no tendency to loop were given several trials to ascertain if their behavior was constant, and then discarded. These animals may have been able to modify their behavior almost immediately, or their failure to exhibit circus movements may have been due to an imperfect covering of the eye, or to the effect of contact stimulus. Of those bees which did perform circus movements, records were taken about every ten to twenty minutes from 9 a.m. to 5 p.M., with the exception of about an hour at noon. Ten bees were thus tested. Four of these animals displayed a steady and marked improvement in the course of the trials. Two others showed some improvement, although considerably less than the first four. Two more of the ten improved for a time, only to regress again, so that while a number of trials near the middle of the series were somewhat modified, those at either end were rouch alike in the number of loops performed. The last two bees showed practically no improvement, although in one of the ani- mals the tendency to circle was at no time very pronounced. It is quite certain, therefore, that at least some bees are able to mod- ify their responses to directive light through experience. The records shown in figure 17 afford a striking example of this modifiability. Although in its first trials the animal looped re- peatedly as it crept toward the light, it was subsequently able to reach the light by a nearly straight course. This animal, how- 398 DWIGHT E. MINNICH O \ ° ; ) ce OLr y O O fe) ) rw oO x [/ | (| Lz \ bled Md. Fig. 17 Records of a bee with the left eye blackened, in directive light, show- ing modifiability of behavior through experience. Alternate records from a series of twenty-six are shown in the figure. PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 399 ever, as well as the others, usually circled again toward the func- tional eye upon reaching the non-directive region near and directly beneath the lamp. The records shown in the figure do. not include this region. Dolley (’16, p. 402) states that he observed some modification from day to day in the behavior of Vanessa in non-directive light. The following evidence seems to indicate that the same is true, to at least some extent, for the honey-bee also. In directive light, individuals with one eye covered were sometimes observed. to begin to swerve toward the functional eye, only to check them- selves by a sharp turn in the opposite direction. Correcting movements of this sort sometimes occurred repeatedly in a single trial, with the result that the animal reached the source of light by a much more direct course than it would have otherwise been able to follow. Precisely the same sharp turns away from the functional eye were occasionally seen in non-directive light also. It seems probable, therefore, that modifiability through experi- ence affects the behavior of bees in non-directive as well as in directive light. 2. Fluctuating factors The variables which have thus far been discussed are those which continuously or progressively affect the behavior of bees throughout an experiment. They probably account in a large measure for such phenomena as the persistant turning toward the covered eye in certain individuals or the apparent lack of any tendency to turn at all in still others. They do not, however, explain the many sudden changes cf behavior which were ob- served. Such variations are dependent upon factors of behavior which fluctuate from time to time. A few of these factors result from environmental changes. The majority, however, arise from changes within the organism itself. a. External. In quantitative experiments every possible pre- caution was exercised to keep all external factors uniform, except the intensity of the light which was changed from trial to trial. This, of course, was possible to a limited extent only. The manipulation of the bees introduced varying mechanical stim- 400 DWIGHT E. MINNICH uli which were quite unavoidable. An animal was accidentally pressed slightly in changing it from one cage to another, or, fail- ing to react to photic stimulation, the cage had to be shaken. Again, in transferring a bee from the light to which it was sub- jected for activation to the center of the non-directive light cham- ber, it was subjected to increases and decreases of light intensity. All of these details and endless others, collectively and individu- ally, were undoubtedly responsible for many of the sudden variations of behavior which occurred. b. Internal. The chief causes of irregular variations, however, are internal. When under constant external conditions, a bee varies the direction of its turning several times in the course of a single trial, the behavior must be attributed to changes within the animal itself, the physiological states of Jennings (’06). Gener- ally speaking, the analysis of these changing physiological states is difficult or impossible. In several instances, however, I was able to make a fairly certain diagnosis. I may cite several examples. Bee no. 147 was subjected to the usual quantitative experi- ment in non-directive light. From the beginning this animal exhibited a rapid, uneasy locomotion. Its entire behaviormay best be described by the word ‘excitable.’ In the course of the first day after the eye was blackened, five pairs of determinations were made. In 24 me. light, the animal circled chiefly toward the covered eye, while in 957 mc., its behavior varied from one set of records to another. On the following morning, the first pair of determinations was begun at 9:49. The bee circled markedly toward the covered eye in both intensities of light. At 12:14, a second pair of determinations was begun. In making the first trial, the bee rushed about its cage for some minutes before finally creeping up into the non-directive light chamber. When it did appear, it seemed greatly excited and crept very rapidly. As the series of records progressed, the animal circled more and more pronouncedly toward the functional eye, the locomotion grew more rapid, and a continuous buzzing began. The performance of small circles toward the functional eye in 24 me. light was surprising, for in PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 401 all previous record sets for this intensity, the animal had shown a more or less pronounced tendency to circle toward the covered eye. In the course of the fourth trial in 957 me. light, the bee had reached a state of intense excitation. Its turning became so rapid, and the consequent circles so small, that at length the animal tumbled over on its back. Defaecation occurred. Mean- while it continued buzzing loudly, and, though lying on its back, managed to whirl round and round toward the functional eye. After the trial just described, the animal was allowed to rest for two hours. At 2:44 and 4:10, respectively, two more pairs of determinations were made. The bee continued to circle markedly toward the functional eye in 957 me. light, and more or less toward the functional eye in 24 me. light. The ‘excitement’ which had characterized the previous trials, however, was absent, and the animal manifested signs of weakness and exhaustion. In the behavior of this animal there was a sudden—even vio- lent—increase of phototropism. This was probably due to an unusual intensification of activity. I have repeatedly observed the close correlation between these two features of behavior in bees. As a rule, the greater the activity, the more pronounced is the manifestation of phototropism. The increase of activity in this animal was produced by a state of metabolism, entailed by a collection of faeces in the intestine. That such’ a condition may affect the activity of bees to a marked degree is evidenced by the following statement made by Phillips and Demuth (’14, p. 12) in connection with a study of certain hive conditions in win- ter. ‘‘It therefore appears that the accumulation of feces (in the intestine) acts as an irritant, causing the bees to become more active and consequently to maintain a higher temperature.” In several other animals defaecation occurred in the course of a trial without being preceded by any conspicuous change of behavior. Neither the amount voided nor the force of expulsion, however, gave any evidence of long accumulation or intestinal irritation in these cases. It seems reasonably certain, therefore, that the sudden increase of phototropism exhibited by bee no. 147 was due to the accumulation of faeces in the intestine. 402 DWIGHT E. MINNICH There were likewise certain other physiological conditions which seemed to intensify photic reactions. Thus bees, upon first recovering from anaesthesia, were frequently observed to creep in very small circles toward the functional eye. Bees, which throughout an experiment appeared physically weak, were also apt to be more intense in their positive deflections. Examples of this latter behavior were afforded by bees nos. 77 and 92. Again, individuals which appeared vigorous at the beginning of an experiment, but became weak and moribund toward the end, generally showed a progressive increase in their circus movements. For example, bee no. 91 circled rather strongly toward the cov- ered eye at first. In the course of the experiment, the animal became weak. Correspondingly, its average deflections became more and more positive until, just before being discarded, it was turning at the rate of +23.82°/cm. in 957 me. light. These instances demonstrate the profound manner in which internal factors are capable of modifying photic behavior. As a rule, only the change in behavior is noted. The recognition of the internal state which conditions this outward expression is possible only in extreme cases. Nevertheless, I believe that these internal factors were directly responsible for most of the sudden variations which characterized the behavior of so many bees. Thus far we have tacitly assumed that the honey-bee is a purely reflex organism. It is not the purpose of the present paper to discuss the mooted question of psychic powers in this animal. It may be said, however, that the opinion advanced by Bethe (’98), that the behavior of bees affords no evidence of psychical attri- butes, has not met with extensive approbation. Forel (’07) and v. Buttel-Reepen (’07), in particular, have presented consider- able evidence to show that bees are something more than mere reflex machines. v. Buttel-Reepen (’07, p. 23) has shown that after bees have been deadened with chloroform, ether, saltpeter, puffball, etc., their memory for location entirely disappears. Subsequently they may again ‘learn’ the position of the hive, etc., but for the time at least, “they have forgotten everything previously known.” My own observations have shown that during recovery from an PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 403 anaesthetic and in weakened or moribund conditions the photic responses of bees become more intense. Photic behavior, how- ever, is probably largely reflex in character. It would appear, therefore, that the same conditions which occasion a loss of ‘memory’ or other central function and the like cause the reflex phases of behavior to appear more boldly. In other words bees, though fundamentally reflex, may possess certain rudiments of higher behavior. Under the influence of narcotics and anaes- thetics or in moribund conditions, these factors cease to affect behavior, and the animal is reduced to a simple reflex condition. If this be correct, we have here an important variable to account for modifications of photic behavior. 3. Summary The variability of respdnse displayed by bees with one eye blackened when creeping in non-directive light is never due to a permanent loss of phototropism or to after-effects of one inten- sity upon trials in a second intensity. It is attributable to the following causes: a. Conditions of temperature and humidity. b. Failure to eliminate completely the photoreceptors on one side of the body. c. Effect of contact stimulus afforded by the eye covering. d. Natural asymmetry of individuals. e. Modifiability through experience. f. Mechanical stimuli attendant upon manipulation. g. Internal factors which affect behavior variously from time to time. VIII. NATURE OF PHOTIC ORIENTATION 1. Theories In recent years the photic behavior of lower animals has been the subject of two theories, respectively known as the ‘continuous action theory’ and the ‘change of intensity theory.’ The ‘con- tinuous action theory,’ as its name implies, postulates a continu- ous action of light upon the organism, orientation resulting when 404 DWIGHT E. MINNICH such action is equal on the symmetrical photoreceptors of oppo- site sides of the body. In its present form this theory is perhaps best summed up by Loeb (’16, p. 259). ‘‘If a positively helio- tropic animal is struck by light from one side, the effect on tension: or energy production of muscles connected with this eye will be such that an automatic turning of the head and the whole animal towards the source of light takes place; as soon as both eyes are illuminated equally the photochemical reaction velocity will be: the same in both eyes, the symmetrical muscles of the body will’ work equally, and the animal will continue to move in this direc- tion. In the case of the negatively heliotropic animal the picture is the same except that if only one eye is illuminated the muscles: connected with this eye will work less energetically.” The ‘change of intensity theory,’ however, accounts for ori- entation in an entirely different manner. According to it, the proc- ess depends not upon the continuous action of light, but upon. the intermittent action of rapid changes in its intensity (Jennings. 04, 706, ’09; Mast, 711). In positive organisms the effective stimulus is assumed to be a sudden decrease of intensity on the photoreceptor; in negative organisms, a sudden increase. A photopositive animal, such as the honey-bee, for example, orients. and maintains its orientation through sudden swervings away from. the side experiencing a decrease of illumination, and orientation. is attained when neither eye is undergoing such a decrease. A similar explanation is applied to photonegative organisms except that the effective stimulus for them is assumed to be an increase of intensity. There is thus a wide diversity in the explanations of orientation offered by these two theories. In the concluding pages of this paper, therefore, I propose to discuss the evidence afforded by my own experiments, as well as that afforded by the observations on circus movements in general, with a view to ascertaining which of the two theories more correctly applies to the orienta- tion of arthropods. PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 405 2. Orientation in the honey-bee As has been previously stated, the process involved in circus movements must be regarded as identical in every respect with that involved in normal orientation. The circus movement is the orienting process. A normal creeping bee may be caused to ‘ perform circus movements without having one eye blackened, if the light is merely moved so as to keep it constantly to one side of the animal. Whether the eye be blackened or the light be moved, the case isthe same. The orienting process is merely pro- longed, and the final attainment: of orientation prevented. The experimental data detailed in the present paper show con- clusively that when one eye of a bee is blackened, the resulting circus movements are produced by the continuous action of the light upon the functional photoreceptor. In the experiments in non-directive light, the only photic stimulation afforded was one of constant, almost uniform intensity over the entire surface of the eye. Under such conditions, the animals not only performed circus movements toward the functional eye, but the amount of turning increased with an increase in the stimulus. It is clear, therefore, that the process of normal orientation, which is iden- tical with that involved in the circus movement, must also be dependent upon the continuous action of light. The impulses arising from this stimulation are, at least in part, transmitted to the musculature of the opposite side of the body, since upon hemisecting the brain, the bee suffers a complete loss of phototropism (Holmes, ’01, p. 227). Although his experi- ments were not conclusive on the point, Holmes believed that the result obtained was not entirely due to ‘“‘the effect of the shock of the operation, or of incidental injury to other paths of photic impulses.’ It must, therefore, have been due to the sev- erance of crossed tracts or commissures which served in the trans- mission of such impulses. There are present in the dorsocerebron of the bee at least three commissures in more or less intimate connection with the optic tracts (Kenyon, ’96), and it is possible that these are the elements concerned. There is thus neurologi- cal as well as physiological evidence for the crossed transmission of photic impulses. THE JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL ZOOLOGY, VOL. 29, No, 3 406 DWIGHT E. MINNICH The resultant effect of these impulses on the opposite side of the body is most probably an increase in the tonus of the extensor muscles. I have no direct evidence on this point in the case of the honey-bee. However, Holmes (’05) and Holmes and McGraw (13), in experiments dealing with unilateral stimulation of photopositive insects, frequently observed that the legs on. the side away from the stimulated eye were strongly extended, while those on the same side exhibited a pronounced flexion. Recently, Garrey (17) has published an account of numerous experiments in which the same phenomenon was observed. In such animals it is clear that orientation is effected through a difference in the posture and not through a difference in the speed of the legs on the two sides of the body. This conclusion is further substantiated by the results obtained by Dolley (’17) on Vanessa. Careful measurement of the velocity of these butter- flies showed that they did not creep faster in a very intense illumination than they did in a fairly weak one. From the above observations, therefore, it is clear that in many insects orienta- tion is effected through changes of tension in the leg muscles. As previously stated, I have not been able to observe any constant and pronounced difference in the muscular tension on the two sides of the body in the honey-bee, although I have made only casual observations in this direction. I do believe that orienta- tion is produced-in this manner, however, and that the failure to detect it was due to the slight degree of the tensions together with the extreme rapidity of their execution. This attempt to analyze the process of orientation in the honey- bee is, of course, far from complete. Certain features of it, how- ever, may be defined with reasonable certainty. Thus it is clear that the stimulus regulating photic orientation is continu- ous and not intermittent. Furthermore, it appears to be essen- tial that at least some of the ‘impulses arising in the eye be transmitted to the opposite side of the body, where they probably regulate the tonus of the extensor muscles. Asfar asit is known, therefore, the process of orientation in the honey-bee is in strict conformity with the ‘continuous action theory.’ PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 407 8. General evidence In experimenting with locomotor organisms, it is not an easy matter to regulate absolutely the conditions of photic stimulation. Thus, a photopositive animal as it moves toward the light is acted upon continuously by the light. But it is also subjected not only to a gradual increase of intensity with every forward movement, but also to sudden decreases and increases with every lateral deviation, however slight. Whether the orientation of the organism, therefore, is effected through a continuous action of the stimulus or only through sudden changes in its intensity from time to time, is frequently difficult to determine with cer- tainty. This difficulty in separating the two conditions experi- mentally has led to much confusion in solving the problem of orientation. Among arthropods, however, there is a growing body of evidence tending to show that orientation is produced by continuous photic stimulation and not by intermittent changes of intensity. No stronger evidence is to be found in this connection than that afforded by the general phenomenon of circus movements. The importance of this evidence has been repeatedly emphasized by Parker (’03, ’07), Loeb (’06, 713), Bohn (’09 a, b), Holmes and McGraw (’18), Garrey (717), and others. Parker (707, p. 548), in reviewing one of the earlier expositions of the ‘change of intensity theory’ (Jennings, ’06), states the case clearly when he says, ‘‘If the modern tropism theory were as weak as Jennings would have us believe, the experimental evidence upon which it rests ought easily to be explained away. Yet it has always seemed to the reviewer that the characteristic circus movements performed by animals immersed in a homogeneous stimulant, but with sense organs unilaterally obstructed, are explainable only on the basis of this theory.” This statement is certainly justified by the facts. Circus movements seem quite incapable of explanation in terms of the ‘change of intensity theory’ of orientation. Let us examine the case of a photopositive arthropod, with the right eye blackened, as it creeps toward a source of light. From experiment we know 408 DWIGHT E. MINNICH that such an individual usually loops to the left. As it does so, however, the functional eye experiences a pronounced decrease of stimulation during the first half of each loop. According to the ‘change of intensity theory,’ such decreases should result in swerves toward the opposite side. Such, however, do not occur, as arule. The animal, instead, completes the loop. It, there- fore, does not respond very strongly to a decrease of intensity. If it did, the performance of circus movements would be quite impossible. If we assume, however, that the looping is produced by the continuous inequality in the stimulation received by the two eyes, the asymmetry of response becomes intelligible at once. For, in an animal with one eye blackened, the functional eye, even when turned farthest from the light, is the recipient of some stimulation, whereas the covered eye receives none whatever. It may be objected, however, that many positive arthropods with one eye blackened are able to overcome their tendency to circle in directive light, and that this is a response to the decrease of intensity on the functional eye at the beginning of each loop. Such may well be the case, as Holmes (’05, p. 345) has suggested. This is a matter for further experiment to decide. In any case, however, circus movements must be regarded as an established phenomenon of general occurrence among this group of animals. The significant thing, therefore, to an understanding of orienta- tion is to discover what is effective in producing these reactions rather than what is effective in modifying them. The evidence afforded by circus movements in directive light as to the nature of the stimulus concerned in their production is thoroughly corroborated by the results obtained in non-directive light. Under the conditions of non-directive illumination em- ployed by Holmes and McGraw (’13) and by myself, an animal is absolutely free from any pronounced or consistent changes in the intensity of the stimulus to which it is subjected. More- over, the experiments of Dubois (’86) on the beetle Pyrophorus furnish a case in which there is no possibility whatever of inten- sity changes playing a significant réle in orientation, since the source of light is within the animal itself. Yet in all these cases the elimination of one eye was usually followed by typical circus PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 409 movements toward the functional eye. In the absence of signifi- cant intensity changes, these responses must have been produced through the continuous action of light. The correctness of this conclusion is further attested by the fact that bees with one eye blackened tend to circle more toward the functional eye in a non- directive light of high intensity than in one of lowintensity. The response may thus be made to vary with the intensity of a con- stantly acting stimulus. Bohn (709, a, ’09 b) has suggested circus movements as a, cri- terion for tropisms. Certainly, if the photic orientation of an animal is the result of a continuous action of the light on both eyes, as the tropism hypothesis postulates, the elimination of one eye should produce circus movements. The form of response will, of course, be subject to the peculiarities of locomotion. However, the elimination of the photoreceptors on one side of the body should result in a more or less asymmetrical response toward or away from that side, depending upon the index of pho- totropism. A failure to obtain such responses means either that the orienting stimulus does not consist in the continuous action of light or that modifying factors are present which interfere with the expected response. As stated in the introductory pages of this paper, the failures to obtain circus movements which have thus far been reported, are, I believe, to be attributed to the lat- ter rather than to the former cause. In conclusion, we may say that circus movements, both in direc- tive and non-directive illumination, are produced by the con- tinuous action of light and not by intermittent changes in its intensity. This, together with the general occurrence of cir- cus movements among arthropods and the close relationship of such responses to normal orientation afford strong evidence that in this group of animals photic orientation is normally produced through the continuous action of light. This does not mean that photosensitive arthropods do not respond to sudden changes in illumination. They undoubtedly do. The orientation of the body toward or away from a source of light, however, cannot be fundamentally the result of such responses. 410 DWIGHT E. MINNICH IX. GENERAL SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS 1. Light exerts a kinetic influence upon the honey-bee; that is, it tends to induce activity. Inits absence, on the other hand, activity is either greatly reduced or entirely lacking. 2. Isolated worker bees, in an active condition, exhibit strong positive phototropism when flying or creeping. Temporary suppressions of this response may occur, however. 3. Normal bees when creeping in non-directive light usually exhibit pronounced asymmetrical responses of constant or varia- ble index. Since essentially the same responses occur in total darkness, they are not fundamentally dependent upon photic stimulation. They are probably, therefore, conditioned largely by internal factors. 4, Bees with one eye blackened usually loop toward the func- tional eye as they creep toward a source of light. Some indi- viduals are found, however, which display little tendency to loop, and occasionally an animal loops toward the covered eye. In the absence of experience, the tendency to loop toward the functional eye remains a permanent feature of behavior. 5. In non-directive light, bees with one eye blackened gener- ally circle toward the functional eye, although a number are ound which circle more or less toward the covered eye. 6. Although subject to considerable variation, bees with one eye blackened tend, in general, to circle more toward the func- tional eye in non-directive light of 957 me. than in one of 24 me. This tendency may be manifested in: a. A lesser amount of turning toward the covered eye in the intense light than in the less intense one. b. A greater amount of turning toward the functional eye in the more intense light than in the less intense one. 7. Since circus movements not only occur in a uniform, non- directive light field, but also vary in amount with the inten- sity of the light, they are produced by continuous unilateral stimulation. 8. In bees with one eye blackened, the rate of locomotion, unlike the amount of turning, is not dénendend upon the intensity of photic stimulation. PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE All 9. The variability of response displayed by bees with one eye blackened, when creeping in non-directive light, is never due to a loss of phototropism or to after-effects of one intensity upon trials in a second intensity. It is attributable to the following causes: a. Conditions of temperature and humidity. b. Failure to eliminate completely the photoreceptors on one side of the body. c. Effect of contact stimulus afforded by the covering of eye. d. Natural asymmetry of individuals. e. Modifiability through experience. f. Mechanical stimuli attendant upon manipulation. g. Internal factors which may affect behavior from time to time, but not necessarily continuously. 10. Photic orientation in the normal honey-bee is effected through the continuous action of light on both photoreceptors. 11. The following considerations afford strong evidence that among arthropods generally, orientation to light is effected through the continuous action of the stimulus rather than inter- mittent changes of its intensity. a. Circus movements are of general occurrence among photo-. tropic arthropods. b. The process involved in circus movements is identical with that involved in normal orientation. c. Circus movements in directive light are explainable only on. the assumption of continuous photic stimulation. d. Circus movements are performed under conditions of non- directive illumination where the only stimulus afforded is one: of approximately constant intensity. 12. Circus movements, as Bohn has suggested, furnish a cri- terion for testing the ‘continuous action theory’ of orientation.. The failure of the test, however, does not necessarily invalidate the theory. Postscript. The preparation of this paper has been much retarded by the absence of the author, who is stillin government service in France. Hence it has not been possible to include im the discussion Garrey’s recent paper (Jour. Gen. Physiol., vol. 1, 412 DWIGHT E. MINNICH p. 118), in which he has shown that the diameter of the circle in circus movements varies with changes in light intensity, nor Loeb’s most recent volume on ‘Forced Movements, Tropisms, and Animal Conduct.’’ X. BIBLIOGRAPHY AXENFELD, D. 1899 Quelques observations sur la vue des arthropods. Arch. ital. de biol., t. 31, pp. 370-376. Berue, A. 1897a Das Nervensystem von Carcinus maenas. Arch. f. mikr. Anat., Bd. 50, 8S. 460-546. 1897 b Vergleichende Untersuchungen tiber die Functionen des Cen- tralnervensystems der Arthropoden. Arch. f. d. ges. Physiol., Bd. 68, 8. 449-545. 1898 Diurfen wir den Ameisen und Bienen psychische Qualitaten zuschreiben? Arch. f. d. ges. Physiol., Bd. 70, 8. 15-100. Boun, G. 1909a La naissance de l’intelligence. Paris. 350 pp. 1909 b Les tropismes. VIme Congrés International de Psychologie. Genéve. Rapports et Comptes rendus, pp. 325-337. Brunpin, T. M. 1913 Light reactions of terrestrial amphipods. Jour. Animal Behavior, vol. 3, pp: 334-352. Burrer-Rezpen, H. v. 1907 Are bees reflex machines? Experimental con- tribution to the natural history of the honey-bee. Translated by Mary H. Geisler. Published by A. I. Root Co., Medina, O. 48 pp. Carpenter, F. W. 1905 The reactions of the pomace fly (Drosophila ampelo- phila Loew) to light, gravity, and mechanical stimulation. Amer. Nat., vol. 39, pp. 157-171. 1908 Some reactions of Drosophila, with special reference to convul- sive reflexes. Jour. Comp. Neur. and Psych., vol. 18, pp. 483-491. Davenport, C. B. 1897 Experimental morphology. Part 1. New York. xiv + 280 pp. Doutey, W. L., JR. 1916 Reactions to light in Vanessa antiopa, with special reference to circus movements. Jour. Exp. Zoél., vol. 20, pp. 357-420. 1917 The rate of locomotion of Vanessa antiopa in different luminous intensities and its bearing on the ‘continuous action theory’ of orienta- tion. Anat. Rec., vol. 11, p. 519. Dusois, R. 1886 Les élatérides lumineux. Bull. de la Soc. Zool. de France, t. 11, pp. 1-275. 1909 Discussion sur les tropismes. VIme Congrés International de Psychologie. Genéve. Rapports et Comptes rendus., pp. 343-344. Forex, A. 1907 The senses of insects. Translated by Macleod Yearsley. Lon- don. xiv + 324 pp. Franpsen, P. 1901 Studies on the reactions of Limax maximus to directive stimuli. Proc. Amer. Acad. Arts and Sci., vol. 37, pp. 185~227. Garrey, W.E. 1917 Proof of the muscle tension theory of heliotropism. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., vol. 3, pp. 602-609. PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 413 Gorzs, J.A.E. 1796 Belehrung tiber gemeinntitzige Natur- und Lebenssachen. Leipzig. 326 pp. Graser, V. 1884 Grundlinien zur Erforschung des Helligkeits- und Farben- sinnes der Tiere. Prag. u. Leipzig. viii + 322 pp. Hapiuy, P. B. 1908 The reaction of blinded lobsters to light. Amer. Jour. Physiol., vol. 21, pp. 180-199. Heros, W.B. 1911 The photic reactions of sarcophagid flies, especially Lucilia caesar Linn. and Calliphora vomitoria Linn. Jour. Exp. Zodl., vol. 10, pp. 167-226. Hxss, C. 19183a Gesichtssinn. Handb. der vergl. Physiol. von Hans Winter- stein, Bd. 4, S. 555-840. 1913 b Experimentelle Untersuchungen tiber den angeblichen Far- bensinn der Bienen. Zool. Jahrb., Abt. f. allg. Zool., Bd. 34, S. 81-106. 1917 New experiments on the light reactions of plants and animals. Translated by Hilda Lodeman. Jour. Animal Behavior, vol. 7, pp. 1-10. Hotmes, 8. J. 1901 Phototaxis in the amphipoda. Am. Jour. Physiol., vol. 5, p. 211-234, 1905 The reactions of Ranatra to light. Jour. Comp. Neur. and Psych., vol. 15, p. 305-349. Hormgs, 8. J., anp McGraw, K. W. 1918 Some experiments on the method of orientation to light. Jour. Animal Behavior, vol. 3, pp. 367-373. Jennines, H.S. 1904 Contributions to the study of the behavior of lower or- ganisms. Carnegie Inst. of Washington, pub. no. 16. 256 pp. 1906 Behavior of the lower organisms. New York. xiv + 366 pp. 1909 Tropisms. VIme Congrés International de Psychologie Genéve. Rapports et Comptes rendus, pp. 307-324. Kenyon, F.C. 1896 The brain of the bee. Jour. Comp. Neur., vol. 6, pp. 133- 210. Lozs, J. 1890 Der Heliotropismus der Thiere und seine Ubereinstimmung mit dem Heliotropismus der Pflanzen. Wirzburg. 118 pp. 1906 The dynamics of living matter. New York. xi + 233 pp. 1913 Die Tropismen. Handb. der vergl. Physiol. von Hans Winter- stein, Bd. 4, 8. 451-519. 1916 The organism as a whole from a physicochemical viewpoint. New York. x + 379 pp. : Lussock, J. 1882 Ants, bees and wasps. New York. xix + 448 pp. Mast, 8. O. 1911 Light and the behavior of organisms. New York. xi + 410 pp. McInpoo, N. E. 1914 The olfactory sense of the honey-bee. Jour. Exp. Zodl., vol. 16, pp. 265-346. . ParRkeR, G. H. 1903 The phototropism of the mourning-cloak butterfly, Van- essa antiopa Linn. Mark Anniversary Volume, pp. 453-468. 1907 Jennings on behavior of lower organisms. Science, New Ser., vol. 26, p. 548. Parren, B. M. 1914 A quantitative determination of the orienting reaction of the blowfly larva (Calliphora erythrocephala Meigen). Jour. Exp. Zoél., vol. 17, pp. 213-280. 414 DWIGHT BE. MINNICH Paruurrs, E. F., anp Demutu, G. S. 1914 The temperature of the honey bee cluster in winter. Bull. U. 8. Dept. Agriculture, no. 93, 16 pp. PraTEav, F. 1887 Recherches expérimentelles sur la vision chez les arthro- podes. Ire partie. Bruxelles. 44 pp. Poucnst, G. 1872 De l’influence de la lumiére sur les larves diptéres privées. d’organs extérieurs de la vision. Rev. et Mag. de Zool., sér. 2, t. 23, pp. 110-117, 129-138, 183-186, 225-231, 261-264, 312-316. RAput, E. 1901 Untersuchungen tiber die Lichtreactionen der Arthropoden. Arch. f. d. ges. Physiol., Bd. 87, 8. 418-466. 1903 Untersuchungen tiber den Phototropismus der Tiere. Leipzig. viii + 188 pp. Srockarp, C. R. 1908 Habits, reactions, and mating instincts of the ‘walking stick,’ Aplopus Mayeri. Papers from the Tortugas Laboratory of the Carnegie Institution of Washington, vol. 2, pp. 45-49. Treviranus, G. R. 1832 Die Erscheinungen und Gesetze des organischen Lebens. Bd. 2, Abt. 1. Bremen. 2384 pp. Turner, C. H. 1912 The reactions of the mason wasp, Trypoxylon albotarsus to light. Jour. Animal Behavior, vol. 2, pp. 353-362. Waurer, H. E. 1907 The reactions of planarians to light. Jour. Exp. Zodl., vol. 5, pp. 35-162. PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE XI. APPENDIX (TABLE 2) 415 A B c D E F a H 1 en Av! En Av." Bosce aw ennce eee -+ VALUES |— VALUES on ne “ornee. NUMBER | poRight| ToLErT |, P=ROM, | PERCM. | OFE—D | OPH D | sxconps | SECONDS OF BEB 057 uc. 957uc, | TURNED 24 |ruRNED 957} ord ord 24 Mo 957 uc. LIGHT Lienn | MO UtGHT| Mc. uIGET LIGHT LIGHT 21 8.02 | +15.84 | +29.41 | 13.57 30 30 10.63 | — 3.00 | +19.33 22.33 30 30 — 3.22] + 2.91 6.13 60 60 — 2.385 | + 3.34 5.69 52 60 + 6.96 | +13.59 6.63 60 60 + 5.06 | — 2.32 7.38 58 60 — 0.59 | + 2.70 3.29 90 80 — 3.62] +14.72 | 18.34 49 47 — 1.58] + 0.53 2.11 70 80 22 0.19 — 7.83 | — 0.74 7.09 30 19 3.00 + 0.96 | — 3.08 4.04 20 22 — 5.51] + 0.40 5.91 30 21 — 4.41 0.00 4.41 16 16 — 3.65 | — 6.37 2.72 60 60 — 0.72 | — 1.54 0.82 42 42 — 5.92] — 7.32 1.40 76 60 — 0.382 | + 1.45 1.77 27 33 23 2.22 | + 9.72 | +13.50 3.78 30 30 10.88 + 8.92 | + 6.44 2.48 60 60 +10.59 | + 7.87 2.72 30 22; + 5.10 | +22.65 | 17.55 63 60 + 0.32 | +11.24] 10.92 60 48 — 1.18] + 9.49 | 10.67 38 29 +.5.71 | +16.33 | 10.62 60 60 + 9.96 | +25.12 |] 15.16 68 47 24 12.45 | + 7.19 | +17.86 | 10.67 30 30 25.58 | +12.80 | +27.01 | 14.21 30 30 + 2.17 | +12.11 9.94 55 60 + 1.77 | +17.73 | 15.96 30 30 — 2.03 | + 4.36 6.39 60 60 — 7.02 0.00 7.02 60 60 —11.80 | — 6.37 5.43 60 60 — 7.08 | — 5.66 1.42 60 60 25 8.34 | +13.49 | + 5.32 8.17 30 30 10.09 | — 5.51 | + 6.56] 12.07 30 21 416 DWIGHT E. MINNICH XI. APPENDIX (TABLE 2)—Continued A B c D E F G H c BBE Av BEE AV apace ay race a -++ VALUES |— VALUES “or nee or mae NUMBER | JO mioar| come: | PEROM. | PEROM. | oF E—D | Or z—D Oe eo | aboroe OF BEE 957 mc. | 957 mc, | TURNED 24 |TURNED 957) ord ord 24 Mc. 957 Mc. TiGHT Lieut | M¢-LicHT | Mc. LIGHT Lieut LIGHT 25 + 1.63] — 7.24 8.87 22 28 — 9.75 | — 3.76 5.99 60 55 — 2.81] — 6.57 3.76 60 70 — 7.27 | — 3.25 4.02 30 30 — 6.90 | — 6.63 0.27 60 70 — 3.68 | No reac- 36 tion 31 5.05 — 7.14] — 3.87 3.27 63 48 5.04 0.00 | + 3.02 3.02 50 45 + 3.53 | + 1.05 2.48 71 74 — 8.35 | + 1.73 5.08 37 45 32 9.34 —13.71 | —12.92 0.79 60 60 12.55 — 9.22 | —13.28 4.06 30 30 —12.17 | — 8.86 3.31 60 60 —14.12 | — 8.85 5.27 60 60 33 5.31 + 1.92 | — 1.80 3.72 78 70 8.59 | + 5.08 | + 7.25 2.17 60 64 — 2.12] + 5.08 7.20 58 58 — 6.89 | + 1.50 8.39 33 29 34 2.34 —19.36 | —21.92 2.56 60 60 7.66 —24.85 | —16.82 8.03 50 60 —18.49 | —16.84 1.65 60 60 —22.05 | —23.68 1.63 60 60 36 0.79 | + 3.08 | + 6.29 3.21 90 90 1.94 0.00 | +11.35 | 11.35 30 35 + 2.04 |} + 8.85 6.81 66 70 — 0.66 | + 2.05 2.71 68 79 41 3.92 + 6.46 | + 5.48 0.98 62 60 0.51 | + 9.77 | 412.34 2.57 56 57 — 2.41] + 4.68 7.04 49 49 + 7.53 | + 7.30 0.23 75 69 42 0.92 + 6.01 | +12.24 6.23 76 70 5.66 | + 4.21 | +13.32 9.11 57 60 PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 417 XI. APPENDIX (TABLE 2)—Continued r B GC D K rr. G H og NORMAL NORMAL = DURATION | DURATION wonnen | Pete. | Deno, [#2seR av. [Blac av.°/+ varues|—vanuns| OFREC | OF REo- o ‘a cM, _ -—_ one | “Ssimc. | Sgro, |ROAw=D 2 lromNep 57/ ond” | ond” | SRLONDS | sumone LIGHT uigor | MC. LIGHT | MC. LIGHT LIGHT LIGHT 42 + 8.71 | +11.20 2.49 90 91 + 8.45 | +10.47 2.02 88 90 + 9.49 | +12.47 2.98 60 60 + 7.73 | +18.95 122 57 60 + 9.03 | +13.70 4.67 86 90 +11.18 | +10.23 0.95 90 90 + 9.34 | +13.48 4.14 90 90 43 2.05 — 9.99 | — 2.55 7.44 60 57 3.17 — 4.44 | —12.61 8.17 76 90 — 8.48 | — 8.43 0.05 90 106 — 9.57 | —11.06 1.49 70 60 44 0.00 + 2.04 | +16.10 14.06 61 76 0.57 | + 1.43 | +13.73 12.30 60 45 + 4.53 | +10.36 5.83 73 70 + 2.05 | + 8.76 6.71 72 70 45 5.20 + 1.97 | + 2.39 0.42 48 60 8.01 + 1.03 | + 3.80 2.77 62 69 No rec- | —11.17 84 ord — 4.68 | — 8.76 4.08 59 60 51 3.41 | — 4.49 | + 1.43 5.92 50 53. 3.75 | — 3.96 | — 0.44 3.52 42 48 + 2.87 | + 5.05 2.18 60 55 — 8.30 | — 5.05 3.25 71 70 52 5.00 | + 6.22 | + 9.06 2.84 91 99 2.35 + 2.37 | + 4.59 2.22 66 74 + 3.30 | + 5.02 1.72 97 111 + 8.44 | + 7.57 0.87 75 76 + 6.94 | +11.07 4.13 71 78 53 2.08 | — 5.65 — 3.41 2.24 59 65 2.61 | — 9.92 | — 3.79 6.13 73 59 — 1.19 | — 6.19 5.00 55 50 — 9.72} — 3.63 6.09 30 a2 54 6.91 — 0.79 | + 8.23 9.02 85 90 «418 DWIGHT E. MINNICH XI. APPENDIX (TABLE 2)—Continued A B Cc D E F G@ H I NORMAL | NORMAL’! ova nyz | onp EYE DURATION | DURATION woupzn | Penem. | penom, | PEACE AV."| BLACK av.°/-+ vabuES|— VALUES! Orpen | omps IN OreEe | BT mo | UBT ae roRNap 24 (rownp $67| "om d ond | otON | O87 ao” LIGHT LIGHT MC. LIGHT MC, LIGHT LIGHT / LIGHT 54 | 3.11 +5.77| + 8.25 | 2.48 51 59 + 8.20] + 6.63 1.57| 50 al + 3.37| + 3.54] 0.17 76 67 +1.70|+ 3.71] 2.01 112 | 105 55 7.69] — 0.35| + 4.36] 4.71 66 55 4.17| + 8.14} + 8.89] 0.75 72 64 + 9.88 | + 7.89 1.99] 84 | 119 + 8.64 | + 6.78 1.86] 80 76 56 0.32 | + 1.50 | —12.86 14.36| 61 64 1.45 | + 1.14] —10.27 11.41] 35 30 — 3.73|+1.83| 5.56 73 69 —10.56 | —18.47 7.91] 100 | 115 62 | 1.38 + 1.55 | + 1.36 0.19] 83 73 2.05 | + 4.54] + 2.98 1.56| 133 | 122 + 1.88} +9.18| 7.30 98 99 + 4.42/ 48.71] 4.29 84 80 +10.73 | +11.61] 0.88 77 63 + 6.05 | + 6.33] 0.28 110 | 110 + 5.79} 410.79 | 5.00 42 40 + 5.58| + 5.87] 0.29 2 | 4 + 7.59 | + 7.45 0.14} 116 | 120 + 4.50 | + 4.47 0.03} 103 99 63 5.58 | + 0.48} 411.83] 11.35 52 49 7.50 +3.94/ 48.56] 4.62 78 72 +11.64 | + 2.61 9.03] 35 38 +10.69 | + 8.45 2.24| 48 50 + 4.13] + 4.50] 0.87 87 74 + 3.04] + 3.41], 0.37 48 61 + 3.59 | +12.00] 8.41 41 30 + 0.18] + 8.74] 8.56 56 60 + 2.99] +9.21] 6.22 50 48 + 7.00| + 9.87] 2.87 46 50 +12.58 | +14.74] 2.16 60 54 + 5.59} + 7.79} 2.20 | 80 85 66 1.82 | + 6.78] + 6.48 0.30} 46 46 PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE XT. APPENDIX (TABLE 2)—Continued 419 D n F G H I NORMAL | NORMAL DURATION | DURATION onpen | Punou, | Wonne [ACE Av-"| auace av.*|4-vacons|~vanoxs| QE RES | OF BE or BEE | To RIGHT | To LER | Teen 4 iepaae OB ad a SECONDS | SECONDS 957 mc, 957 mc. 24 uc. 957 mc. LIGHT tient | MC. LIGHT | Mc. LIGHT LIGHT LIGHT 66 0.18 | + 6.46 | + 4.07 2.39 85 93 + 1.73 | + 4.27 2.54 68 68 + 4.36 | + 8.27 3.91 54 55 + 7.40 | + 8.68 1.28 96 86 + 9.45 | + 5.87 3.58 60 60 + 5.28} + 4.93 0.30 142 136 + 4.98 | + 4.60 0.38 100 104 + 3.02 | +11.09 8.07 83 80 + 5.59 | +10.32 4.73 101 122 68 6.08 + 4.35 | +13.12 8.77 97 94 1.94 +11.65 | +16.40 4.75 66 67 + 5.56 | +14.91 9.35 54 59 + 8.97 | +11.23 2.26 60 69 + 8.83 | + 9.82 0.99 87 100 +10.57 | +12.21 1.64 53 63 + 5.62 | +12.06 6.44 75 60 +10.48 | +13.52 3.04 75 87 + 5.32 | +11.79 6.47 91 99 + 4.37 | + 4.76 0.39 100 104 72 5.73 + 7.13 | + 9.11 1.98 60 60 7.30 + 1.02 | +24.35 | 23.33 20 35 + 2.65 | — 0.29- 2.94 77 58 | + 0.87 | + 3.13 2.26 87 86 — &.92 | — 0.66 3.26 103 100 + 6.77 | + 6.22 0.55 120 120 +10.17 | +12.94 2.77 100 91 73 4.94 |°+ 0.92} + 8.27] 7.35 39 40 2.24) + 1.53 | + 4.22 2.69 34 34 — 1.42] +138.04 14.46 60 65 + 2.45 | +10.89 8.44 100 100 + 2.00 | +12.55 10.55 61 75 + 3.29 | +13.79 | 10.50 93 101 + 2.88 | + 9.49 6.61 104 87 — 0.12] — 8.89 9.01 147 147 + 6.07 | +13.82 7.75 135 136 — 1.69 | +15.55 17.24 110 110 420 DWIGHT E. MINNICH XI. APPENDIX (TABLE 2)—Continued A B xed D E Fr G H I NORMAL, | NORMAL | ong mye | ONE EYE TURATION DURATION womper | per em. | ron exi, PLAC AV./BLACK AY."/+ VALUBS)— VALUES) Oppsin | onDS IN OF RES eh oruint Oe ate, TURNED 24 |TURNED 957 ord ord “OA ae. UBT me LIGHT. Liga MC, LIGHT | MC, LIGHT LIGHT LIGHT 77 4,22 +10.71 | +14.43 3.72 57 75 7.46 + 9.62 | +16.59 6.97 88 65 +13.53 | +18.39 | 4.86 48 40 +12.43 | +14.74] 2.31 104 80 + 8.45 | +14.92] 6.47 90 80 +11.64 | +16.89 5.25 120 130 +12.60 | +15.38 2.78 75 70 +15.29 | +17.98 | 2.69 40 35 +11.57 | +18.36 6.79 80 80 81 2.09 + 4.21] +10.60] 6.39 114 117 2.59 + 1.03 | + 3.98 2.95 53 53 + 6.03 | + 7.78 1.75 116 106 + 5.76|+ 5.86} 0.10 108 101 + 3.30 | + 8.35 5.05 70 65 + 4.59 | + 9.89 5.30 56 61 + 5.03 | + 7.49 2.46 63 63 + 4.81 | +13.52 | 8.71 57 80 + 2.76 | + 8.21 5.45 110 122 + 3.71 | +12.60 8.89 67 67 82 5.81 | — 3.94] — 2.38 1.61 124 119 0.65 | — 5.37 | — 5.01 0.36 110 123 —10.30 | — 6.43] 3.87 60 60 — 6.95 | — 4.88] 2.07 59 59 — 2.59] + 1.42] 4.01 66 64 — 3.29 | ~ 0.57 2.72 74 85 — 7.06} + 2.46] 9.52 54 56 — 6.40 | — 5.29 1.11 79 80 — 0.80 | — 5.47 : 4.67 108 105 83 3.59 + 5.62 | +15.93 | 10.31 81 76 5.85 + 8.87 | +15.73| 7.36 90 90 + 8.89 | +10.53 1.64 104 104 + 6.93} 411.49] 4.56 71 71 +12.84 | +21.53] 8.69 58 60 + 9.39 | +17.47| 8.08 60 60 + 4.93 | +16.39 | 11.46 94 90 + 4.53} 412.50] 7.97 41 33 + 3.52 | +14.42 | 10.90 133 128 PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 421 XI. APPENDIX (TABLE 2)—Continued A B c n B PF G a I NORMAL | NORMAL DURATION| DURATION NUMBER oo ol ae ng BLACK AV.” BLACK Av.[+ VALUES|— VALUES Sans aoa OF BEE | TO RIGHT| TO LEFT metre fh romp 687 OF aaa > oe ed D| srconps | SECONDS 957 mc, | 957 mc. 24 mc. 957 mc. LIGHT ticnr | MC. UIGHT | Mc. LIGHT LIGHT LIGHT 85 0.84 | + 6.46 | +12.52 6.06 47 46 0.93 | + 1.41 | + 7.78 6.37 118 117 + 7.08 | + 3.74 3.34 75 82 + 6.87 | + 6.42 0.45 80 108 + 4.34 | + 3.54 0.80 126 123 + 1.73 | + 3.63 1.90 64 74 + 4.61 | + 8.19 3.58 137 123 + 6.13 | + 7.45 1.32 94 94 + 4.68 | + 7.99 3.31 71 58 91 0.48 — 2.42 | — 4.98 2.56 125 125 6.99 —10.05 | — 4.00 6.05 85 80 — 6.44] — 6.05 0.39 95 95 — 7.33 | — 3.05 4.28 81 81 + 2.18 | + 3.52 1.34 110 131 + 0.73 | + 6.69 5.96 64 64 +10.19 | +27.54 | 17.35 30 30 92 10.66 | + 4.42] + 5.77 1.35 113 104 ‘ 8.67 | + 4.28 | + 9.86 5.58 58 62 + 7.41 | + 9.19 1.78 94 85 + 9.64 | + 7.64 : 2.00 63 61 + 7.49 | +17.34 9.85 52 52 +14.19 | +23.82 9.63 60 60 93 0.21 | + 1.61 |} + 6.48 4.87 120 120 2.44 + 0.19} + 1.42 1.23 99° 99 + 2.87 | — 0.46 3.33 63 63 + 2.37 | + 2.92 0.55 98 87 95. 2.29} + 2.14 | + 4.89 2.75 118 128 6.52 | + 1.13 | + 3.07 1.94 100 109 0.00 | + 5.67 5.67. 58 46 + 0.32 | + 6.58 6.26 71 80 + 1.29 | + 4.82 3.53 70 78 + 0.93 | +11.94]) 11.01 34 30 + 2.39 | + 7.18 4.79 69 74 + 3.67) + 7.11 3.44 66 67 + 0.85 | + 6.35 5.50 82 75 + 0.21 | + 1.93 1.72 61 60 THE JOURNAL OF EXPERIMENTAL ZOOLOGY, VOL. 29, NO.3 422 DWIGHT E. MINNICH XI. APPENDIX (TABLE 2)—Continued B NORMAL BEE AY.° ¢c NORMAL BEE AY.° D ONE EYE E ONE EYE F G H DURATION OF REC- I DURATION OF REC- BLACK AY.°| BLACK AV.° |+ VALUES |~ VALUES forsee |7o some) to tars | TER OM,, |, Pen oM.,| O12? | OB |? | suconns | spconps LIGHT Licey MC. LIGHT | MC. LIGHT LIGHT LIGHT 96 2.76 | +10.46 | +19.08) 8.62 90 92 5.54 + 9.29] + 8.50 0.79} 195 192 +11.89 | +15.99] 4.10 60 60 +14.09 | +17.84] 3.75 60 60 + 5.33 | +10.15| 4.82 35 29 101 7.11] — 5.47] — 2.55] 2.92 62 63 6.10 | — 1.67 | — 2.07 0.40 88 79 — 2.09 | — 0.61 1.48 60 59 — 1.02} — 1.00] 0.02 67 70 | — 3.34/ — 1.58] 1.76 46 49 102 3.67 — 1.58 | +14.37 | 15.95 49 49 5.30 + 2.98 | +15.02 | 12.04 50 54 + 3.75] +11.08| 7.33 71 78 — 2.23) + 1.45] 3.68 62 66 — 0.32] + 6.46] 6.78 64 53 — 3.38 | +11.11 | 14.49 57 58 — 1.98} + 4.52] 6.50 41 60 — 2.23 | +10.19 | 12.42 88 88 + 3.55 | +13.35] 9.80 95 95 — 1.28} +12.23 | 13.51 70 60 1038 0.00 — 2.46 | +13.48 | 15.94 57 57 6.34 — 4,29] +11.25 | 15.54 17 60 + 0.16} + 8.49] 8.33 60 69 — 1.26 | +11.36 | 12.62 61 61 — 0.44] +215! 2.59 84 84 — 3.75 | +11.04] 14.79 60 60 + 3.89] + 6.47] 2.58 60 65 + 2.27|/+ 6.99] 4.72 63 68 — 0.18/ + 0.22] 0.40 105 104 + 0.86 | + 4.22] 3.36 62 64 105 5.93 + 8.46 | +14.64] 6.18 68 60 8.81 °49.19 |] +13.78| 4.59 50 60 + 6.89 | +14.10] 7.21 80 80 + 6.55 | +15.20] 8.65 60 60 + 6.74 | +14.83 | 8.09 93. 93 + 6.21 | +12.73} 6.52 60 60 +10.34 | +16.05]} 5.71 60 60 PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 423 XI. APPENDIX (TABLE 2)—Continued A B Cc D Bb F fed H I NORMAL NORMAL ONE BYE ONE EYE , DURATION| DURATION numer | Poros. | Pen cm, |2EACK AY.°| BLACK AV.°[+ vaLuns|— VALUES! Oposty | ons IN OF BEE vO mee er te. TURNED 24|rurNED 957/ ord ord eras ace LIGHT LIGHT MO; LIGHT MO. LIGHT LIGHT LIGHT 105 +10.08 | +17.05 | 7.02 60 60 + 9.04] +11.01} 1.97 100 100 + 7.46| +12.68| 5.22 163 163 106 6.43 —1.18| +1.49| 2.67 70 68 1.71 + 0.44/ + 3.22] 2.78 81 79 + 2.79| + 6.72] 3.93 41 41 — 1.49|4+5.19| 6.68 35 35 +0.59| — 5.49] 4.90 77 77 + 0.53) + 5.33] 4.80 108 115 121 1.61 + 2.96 | +24.04} 21.08] . 60 60 6.87 | +11.57 | +19.71 | 8.14 60 60 + 3.13 | 415.11 | 11.98 92 88 + 1.91 | +12.12] 10.21 87 91 — 4.71] +17.50 | 22.21 60 60 122 2.31 + 1.88} + 8.96] 7.08 69 73 2.98 + 3.65|+ 3.77] 0.12 118 111 ‘ + 3.25 | + 1.99 1.26 90 85 — 4.39] + 6.50] 10.89 56 55 123 17.86 | + 7.16 | +18.55 | 11.39 54 60 3.60 | + 1.79 | — 1.21 4.00 65 69 —10.76 | + 0.59) 11.35 69 67 — 8.08 | + 2.29] 10.37 79 79 = 7,61 |= 2.14) $47 60 60 — 3.46| +1.12| 4.58 140 135 — 3.25|+ 5.69] 8.94 60 58 — 3.5314 0.56] 4.09 58 58 — 2.21] + 1.97] 4.18 90 101 124 2.59 | + 3.88 | +11.18] 7.30 73 68 3.00| + 3.94| + 6.45] 2.51 53 59 +2.54/ 4+ 8.38] 5.84 79 71 +10.36 | +11.19 | 0.83 80 80 411.04 | +17.61 |. 6.57 90 90 +10.39 | +17.41 | 7.02 60 60 + 5.81 | +12.54] 6.73 58 58 + 6.55|+ 8.95] 2.40 74 60 + 7.67 | +10.19| 2.52 60 60 424 DWIGHT E. MINNICH XI. APPENDIX (TABLE 2)—Continued A B c D EB F G Ho I NORMAL _ NORMAL ONE EYE ONE EYE : DURATION: DURATION womngn | ren en. | pon ox. |PEACE AV.” [Lack AV."|-+-vaLuES |—VALUES| Of pein | onpe IN OF BE | TO RIGHT TOTBFT |qunnep id (roman 057 ond | ond | SECONDS | SECONDS tient | ier | MC-MGET | Mc. LicHT LIGHT LIGHT 126 6.51 | — 1.389 | + 3.11 4.50 105 100 1.19 | + 4.51 | — 2.89 7.40 48 50 + 3.73 | + 6.14 2.41 78 77 — 1.44 |°+23.37 | 24.81 45 45 + 1.83 | + 4.41 2.58 107 107 + 4.57 |} + 3.65 0.92 148 - 153 + 5.51 | + 4.32 1.19 72 72 — 2.98 | — 2.26 0.72 83 83 — 1.49 | + 3.73 5.22 25 28 133 2.82 + 2.39 | + 3.18 0.79 70 74 5.86 + 1.98 | + 3.26 1.28 69 68 + 1.54} + 4.31 2.77 60 61 + 4.79 | + 9.29 4.50 60 60 + 1.76 | + 4.92 3.16 82 76 + 3.30 | +11.54 8.24 38 40 + 3.35 | + 7.77 4.42 64 68 134 1.52 | + 8.29 | +19.62 | 11.33 69 69 1.22 | + 9.94 | +19.60 9.66 57 57 + 5.67 | +18.19 | 12.52 80 80 + 0.22 | +18.93 18.71 60 60 + 3.83 | +12.53 8.70 58 58 135 10.77 | — 3.25 | — 0.54 2.71 63 61 10.88 | + 0.74 | + 2.03 1.29 95 89 + 1.29 | + 3.35 2.06 109 109 — 9.49 | — 5.85 3.64 58 59 + 2.28 | — 1.37 4.65 71 71 — 1.05 | — 3.08 1.98 94 94 — 0.33 | — 7.67 7.34 79 76 + 0.44 | — 6.66 7.10 58 59 137 5.99 | +11.73 | +19.09 | 17.36 60 60 2.94 | +15.82 | +16.09 0.27 49 49 +10.59 | +16.45 5.86 70 70 +15.78 | +16.57 0.79 60 60 +14.31 | +15.40 1.09 78 78 +14.71 | +22.57 7.86 60 60 +14.69 | +17.02 2.33 70 70 PHOTIC REACTIONS OF HONEY-BEE 425 XI, APPENDIX (TABLE 2)—Concluded A B c D E F a H 1 NORMAL | NORMAL DURATION | DURATION ues BEUAYs ince te suena? gic are + VALUES |— VALUES paltohade pe nate oF BEB | TO RIGHT | TOLEFT |acuwnp 24 (runNep $57; ond | ond | SBCONDS | SECONDS nicer | ricut | MO. UIGHT | Mc. LicHT LIGHT LIGHT. 137 +13.38 | +15.64 2.26 60 60 + 8.43 | 4-16.71 8.28 60 60 +15.92 | +18.08 2.16 60 60 138 6.55 | — 0.18 | — 4.67 4.49 129 129 8.37 | — 0.37 | + 0.13 0.50 62 58 — 1.12 | +138.52 14.64 65 60 — 9.00 | — 3.52 5.48 70 70 — 3.07 | + 1.60 4.67 115 115 — 1.97 | + 2.57 4.54 77 74 — 6.19 | — 1.08 5.11 69 69 CONTRIBUTIONS FROM THE ZOGLOGICAL LABORATORY OF THE MUSEUM OF COMPARATIVE ZOOLOGY AT HARVARD COLLEGE. ( Continued.) ¢ E. L. Mark, Director. *,* Abbreviations used :— 27T. 278, 279. 280. 281. 282. 283. 284. 285. 286. 287. 288. 289. 290. 291. 292. ° 293. 294. 295. 296. 297 = 298, 299, B.M.C.Z. .. 4446. «for Bull. Mus, Comp. Zoil. PALA. «4.0... 6... for Proceed. Amer. Acad. Arts and Sci. P.B.S.N.H...... .. « . for Proceed. Bost. Soc. Nat. Hist. Norn. — Copies of the List of Contributions, Numbers 1-276, wilt be sent on application. Wennicu, D. H.— Notes on the Reactions of Bivalve Mollusks to Changes in _ Light Intensity: Image Formation in Pecten. Jour. Anim. Behavior, 6 (4): 297-318. July—Aug., 1916. Anrzgy, L..B.—The Influence of Light and Temperature upon the Migration o1, the Retinal Pigment of Planorbis trivolvis. Jour. Comp. Neurol., 26 (4) :359- 389, 1 pl. Aug., 1916. Watton, A. C.— Reactions of Paramoecium caudatum to Light. Jour. Anim. Behavior, 6 (5) :335-340. Sept.-Oct., 1916. . Wauron, A.C.—The ‘Refractive Body’ and the ‘Mitochondria’ of Ascaris canis Werner. Proc. Amer. Acad. Arts and Sci., 52 (5) : 253-266, 2 pls. Oct., 1916. ‘ Parxer, G. H., anv Titus, E. G. — The Structure of Metridium (Actinoloba marginatus Milne-Edwards with Special Reference to its Neuro-muscular Mechanism. Jour. Exp. Zodl., 21 (4) :488-459, 1 pl. Nov., 1916. Parker, G. H.— The Effector Systems of Actinians. Jour. Exp. Zodl., 21 (4): 461-484. Nov., 1916. Watton, A. C.— A Case of the Occurrence of Ascaris triquetra Schrank in Dogs. Jour. Parasitol., $ (1):39-41. Sept. [Noy.], 1916. Parxer, G. H.— Nervous Transmission in the Actinians. Jour. Exp. Zodl., 22 (1) 87-94. Jan., 1917. : PaRkeER, G. H.—The Movements of the Tentacles in Actinians. Jour. Exp ZLodls, 22 (1) :95-110. Jan., 1917. ; Parker, G. H.— Pedal Locomotion in Actinians. Jour. Exp. Zodl., 22 (1) :111- 124, Jan., 1917. Parxer, G. H.— The Responses of Hydroids to Gravity. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., 3 (2):72-73. Feb., 1917. Coz, W. H.—The Reactions of Drosophila ampelophila Loew to Gravity, Cen- trifugation, and Air Currents.. Jour. Anim. Behavior, 7 (1) :71-80. Jan., 1917. Oumstep, J. M. D.— Geotropism in Planaria maculata.:, gour. Anim. Behayior, ¥ (1):81-86. ‘Jan. 1917. PaRgER, G. H.— Actinian Behavior. Jour. Exp.’ ZoGl., 22 (2) :1938-229. Feb., 1917. REDFIELD, E.S. P.—The Rhythmic Contractions of the Mantle of Lamellibranchs. Jour. Exp. Zo6l., 22 (2) : 231-239. Feb., 1917. ReEvriep, A. C.— The Reactions of: the Melanophores of the. Horned Toad. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., & (8) : 202-203. Mar., 1917. REDFIELD, A. C. —The Codrdination of the Melanophore Reactions of the Horned Toad. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., $ (3) : 204-205. Mar., 1917. Porz, P. H.—The Introduction of West Indian Anurainto Bermuda. Bull. Mus. Comp. Zoul., 61 (6) :117-131,'2 pls. May, 1917. Van Heusen, A. P.—The Skin of the Catfish (Amiurus nebulosus) as a Re- ceptive Organ for Light. Amer. Jour. Physiol., 44 (2) :212-214. Sept., 1917. Parker, G. H., anp VAN Heusen, A. P. —The Responses of the Catfish, Amiurus nebulosus, to Metallic and Non-metallic Rods. Amer. Jour. Physiol., 44 (3): 405-420. Oct., 1917. , Parker, G.H.—The Pedal Locomotion of the Sea-Hare Aplysia californica. Jour. Exp. Zobl., 24 (1) :139-145. Oct., 1917. Parker, G. H., anD Van Heusen, A. P.—The Reception of Mechanical Stimuli by the Skin, Lateral-Line Organs and Ears in Fishes, especially in Amiurus. Amer. Jour. Physiol., #4 (4) : 463-489. Nov., 1917. Parker, G. H. — The Power of Suction in the Sen-AnemoneCribrina. Jour. Exp. Zobl., 2H (2) : 219-222. Nov., 1917. CONTRIBUTIONS FROM THE ZOQLOGICAL LABORATORY OF THE MUSEUM OF COMPARATIVE ZOOLOGY AT HARVARD COLLEGE. (Continued.) 300. Parker, G. H,—The Activities of Corymorpha, Jour. Exp. Zodl., 2£ (2) :303- 831. Nov., 1917. 301. Stringer, C. E.—The Means of Locomotion in Planarians. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., 3 (12) :691-692. Dec., 1917. 302, Oumstep,J.M. D.—The Regeneration of Triangular Pieces of Planaria maculata. A Study in Polarity. Jour. Exp. ZoGl., 2% (1) :1574176. Feb., 1918. 303. ; Hecut, S. —The Physiology of Ascidia atra Lesueur. I. General Physiology. { IL. Sensory Physiology. Jour. Exp. ZoGl., 2% (1) :229-299. Feb., 1918, 805. Heout,S.—Id. ILL. The Blood System. Amer. Jour. Physiol., #5 (3) : 157-187. Feb., 1918. 306. Bray, A. W. L.—The Reactions of the Matehephared of Amiurus to Light and Adrenalin. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., # (3) : 58-60. Mar., 1918. 307. Watton, A. C.— The Odgenesis and Early Embryology of Ascaris canis Werner. Jour. Morvh., 30 (2) :627-603. Mar., 1918. : 308. Oxmsrup, J. M. D.— Experiments on the Nature of the Sense of Smell in the Common Catfish, Amiurus nebulosus (Lesueur), Amer. Jour. Physiol.,, 46 (5) :443-458. Aug., 1918. 309. REDFIELD, A. C.—The Physiology of the Melanophores ot the Horned Toad, Phrynosoma. Jour. Exp. Zodl., 26 (2) :275-333. July, 1918. 810. Brooxs, E. 8.— Reactions of Frogs to Heat and Cold. Amer. Jour. Physiol., 46 (5) 7493-501. Aug., 1918. 311. Coss, P. H.— Autonomous Responses of the Labial’ Palps of Anodonta. Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., 4 (8) :234-285, Aug., 1918. 312, Inwin, M.—The Nature of Sensory Stimulation by Salts. Amer. Jour. Physiol., 4&7 (2) :265-277. Nov., 1918. ; : 318. Parker, G. H. — The Rate of Transmission in the Nerve Net of the Coelenterates. Jour. Gen. Physiol., 1 (2) : 231-236. Nov., 1918. 314. Brenuy, A. J.—The Effect of Adrenin on thé Pigment Migration in the Melano- phores of the Skin and in the Pigment Cells of the Retina of the Frog. Jour. Exp. Zoél., 27 (8): 391-396. Jan., 1919. 315. Jorpan, H.— Concerning Reissner’s Fiber in Teleosts. Jour. Comp. Neurol., 3O (2):217-227, 1 pl. Feb., 1919. 316. PaRkER, G. H.—The Organization of Renilla. Jour. Exp. Zodél., 27 (4): 499- 607. Feb., 1919. 317. Hunt, H. R.— Regenerative Phenomena following the Removal of the Digestive Tube and the Nerve Cord of Tash, Bull. Mus. Comp. Zodl., 62 (15): 569-581, 1 pl. Apr., 1919. 318. Davis, D. W.— Asexual Multiplication and Regeneration in Sagartia luciae Verrill. Jour. Exp. Zo6l., 28 (2):161-204. May, 1919. 319. Parker, G. H.— The Effects of the past Winter on the Occurrence of Sagartia luciae Verrill. Amer. Nat., 53 (626): 280-281. May-June, 1919. 320. Mrnnicu, D. E.—The Photic Reactions of the Honey-Bee, Apis mellifera Tu. Jour. Exp. Zo6l , 29 (3) :348-425. Nov., 1919. YL. Mart,’ ree a “et Abbreviations used :- a > 7 "BM. c. ree aes carla . for Bull, Mua. Comp. Zot ae 4 BCAA opie as oe Gee ". for Proceed, Amer. Acad. Arts and Se cet . PyB.SNLH,. a o Bost. Soe, Nat. Hint, ema . aes - Copies 0 of the’ List or Contributions, ‘Wiarader’ 1-276, whit, be a ae ane Ie et on application. ° grr: ‘Wannien, DH — Notes: on the: ‘Renoticns of Bivalve: Mottns to cunnaee ‘in 2’ Eight Intensity: Image Formation: in Pegten., Tour. Ablin: Behavior. 6 ae e" QO7-818. Jaly- AUB» ‘1016.’ i. AngyiL. Bow The Influence of Light and Temperdtu re upon the. Migration, o1 “ Retinal Piginent of Planorbi nivolvis, d fours Comp, N aural. a a0 @): ee : 889, 1 pl... Aug. 1016. ey . an , a. ‘Watton, A. C.— Reactions of “Patetnbeckuge qutlitocy to Light. ‘| Behavior, 6. (6).: 336-840, Sept,-Cet. #1916, » : dso. ‘Wauren, A. Cae The ' Refractiv ‘Rody ond the Miedo at of Ascaris ; iP > oh canis Werner.” Proc. “Amer. Acad. ala and Sci., me (6): 252-266, a A pls. Oct, aeeet ae) AND! foe, B @: ‘the friuseuve of Meteidinm (Actnoloba ° . Mhrginatus Miine-Hdwards with Spécial: ‘Referérice to its Nenro- musculat » ) Mechunisth. . Jour, Exp. 200), 21, (4) :438-459,'1 p}. Nov., 1916. . f 383... PARKER, G. HR “The Eftector§ psteins of Sa hoe Jour. Exp. Zobl., ax (@): _ Nav., 1916. = : ‘A. Oy ACaan of the aA sauvaitte of ‘Avearty elqusien Schrank i in Doge: r- Patasitol., & (1): 39-41, Rept. [Nov.], 1916. fo * 984, Paes, G. H. —Neryous Tra smission jan Actinia MSs Jour. Exp. Zot 2 ~ (1): 8704.5 Tan, 19% 285. PARKER, G. te B00, PARRER, G. Hw The Activities a Conymorptis- Jour. Exp. Baitiy 24 2): 2808 . B81. -Noy., 1917. $01, Sretesr, C. B.: ~The Means of Locomotion in i Plana. “ Proe. Nat. Kead., c Sci., & (12):691-692,. Dee 1. 2. "302, Ouustan, J: M.D. —The- -Regenoration of eeteriee Pieces: of Plainaria wedulater AStudy in Polarity. Jour. Exp. Zodl., 2 (1)+ 157-176... Feb., 1918+. 808, ‘Hot, 8. —The Physiology of ‘Ascidia atra Lesacuzz. I. General Physiology. 304, IL. Sensory Physiology. Jour. Exp. Zoél., 25 a): 1229-299, Febs, 1918, 305., y, NCEE: —Id.- i The Blood uel Amer, Jéur een 45 ee 18187. = ~ Feb., 1018, : we-iliearistte. Broa: Nat. Acad: Sci., 4 (3): 58-80. —" 807, Watton, A. C.—The Odgenesis and-Early Embryology, “of Ascaris canis Werner. Jour«.Morvh., $0 (2): : 6276603. — Mar., 1918, ~ man - 308. Onmstup, J. M. D.— ~ Experimente. on the Nature ‘of ‘the Sense of Smell i in the? al Common ‘Catfish, Amiurus nebulostis (Lesneur),.: “Amer. Tour: Physiol., 46 ca (6) 448-458. Alig., 2018. < ae 309. -Repried, A. C.—The “Physiology of the Malanoptiores ot the Hoimed. Tondy’ oe ‘Phrynosoma. Jour. Exp. Zoi, 26 (2), +276-383. July, 1918. -- aia dour: Physiol, 310, BRooxs}.E. S.— Reactions ot Frogs, to~Heat and ‘Cold. Amer - "46.5 1498-50L Aug. 1918 7 . ; 311, Cops, P. H.— Autonomous Responses of the bial Palps of Anodonta. Pres Nab. Avad. Sci, & (8) : 234-285. “Aug., 1918.” - oo 312, IRWIN: M.—Thé Nature: of § Sensory’ Stimulation. by Salts, Amer, J our. Pry ae ag (2): 265-277. Nov., 1918.’ 313. _PaRKur, G.H.— The Rate of ‘Eransmission in the Nerve es of the Cosfentratek? : 7 Jour.- -Gén, Physiol; 1 (2) : 231-236. “Nov., 1918. are B14 Sums A. J.=The Effect of Adrenin on the Pigment Migration in the- Melano-* = © phoreé of th Skin and in the Pigment. Cells of the Retina. of the Frog: Jot eg “Exp. Zoéls, 27 (3): 391-306. ‘Tan. 1919, i ae 315. Jorpan, H.— Concerning Reigsner’s* ‘Fiber i Peleosts. ‘Shite. Goimp. Neato, Ee BO (2);217-227, 1pl. Feb. 1919-2 : - 316. Parker, G. H. ~The ‘Org Organiza\ jon of Rehilla. Tour. Exp. Lost. are: 499 .. 607. “Feb, 1919, eae sy 817. Hunt, H. R.— -~ Regenerative Phenomena following the Reiioval of he: Digestiy ; o Tube and the Nerve Cord of Earthworms. Ball. Mas. Comp. adel -@2 (15) Te 669-5814 pl. Apr.; 1919. . we ’ 5 vee Davis,’ D: W.— As - Verrill. Sout Exp. Zobl., 28 (2): ‘161-204, May, “119. "819. _ PAREER, G.H.- +The Eifects of the past Winter on. the Ocgurrénce Sage 7, taeiae¥ erill Amer. Nat, 58 (8 ) 280-281. “May-June, 1919, 820, Minwice, D. E.—The Photic Réactions of the ‘Honey-Bee, Apis -Jour. Exp, Zool, 29 (8): $343-425.° Nov., 1919.