ye AA ppebihitabbad pe Ribot ebes bith ui Ny {ut ! iy ee eee rane t-te CORNELL UNIVERSITY © LIBRARY GAYLORD Cornell University Libra Heredity and eugenics HEREDITY AND EUGENICS THE UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO PRESS CHICAGO, ILLINOIS gents THE BAKER & TAYLOR COMPANY NEW YORE THE OAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS LONDON AND EDINBURGH HEREDITY AND EUGENICS A COURSE OF LECTURES SUMMARIZING RECENT ADVANCES IN KNOWLEDGE IN VARIATION, HEREDITY, AND EVOLUTION AND ITS RELATION TO PLANT, ANIMAL, AND HUMAN IMPROVE- MENT AND WELFARE BY WILLIAM ERNEST CASTLE JOHN MERLE COULTER CHARLES BENEDICT DAVENPORT EDWARD MURRAY EAST WILLIAM LAWRENCE TOWER THE UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO PRESS CHICAGO, ILLINOIS COPYRIGHT 1912 By Tue UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO All Rights Reserved Published June 1912 Second Impression January I913 Composed and Printed By The University of Chicago Press Chicago, Mlinois, U.S.A. PREFACE During the summer of rg11, a course of lectures on heredity and allied topics was given at the University of Chicago, under the auspices of the biological departments. The purpose of the course was to present the recent develop- ments of knowledge in reference to variation, heredity, and evolution, and the application of this new knowledge to plant, animal, and human development and improvement. The lectures were not intended for those trained in biology, but for a general university audience, interested in the progress of genetics as a matter of information rather than of study. The lecturers, therefore, did not address themselves to their colleagues, and did not attempt to include any considerable amount of new material. It is believed that a much larger audience than the one originally addressed might be interested in this summary of results in one of the important and recently cultivated fields of biology, and therefore this volume has been published. It is hoped that it may perform a service not only for those interested in biology as a field outside their own experience, but also for those biologists whose work deals with other phases of biology. The lectures were given by five lecturers, with no oppor- tunity to relate the lectures to one another other than as suggested by the assigned titles. It is inevitable that there should be more or less overlapping of statements, and no attempt has been made to avoid this. Each lecture, therefore, is complete in itself, as it was delivered. Vv vil Preface No attempt has been made to include the whole of the fields represented by the general topics. The plan was to select certain representative investigators to speak of their work. Four such investigators were selected, the mission of the fifth lecturer being to give the elementary information (chaps. 1 and ii) necessary for an audience untrained in biology, and thus to prepare the way for the more special topics. It is hoped that similar series of lectures in other fields of biology will be given during successive summers, and that the present volume may be the first of a series which will represent the most significant aspects of current biological investigation. J. M. CouLtTEerR fale at By 0 2 83 W. L. Tower CHAPTER I E III. IV. VI. VI. VIII. IX. TABLE OF CONTENTS RECENT DEVELOPMENTS IN HEREDITY AND EVOLUTION: GENERAL INTRODUCTION Tue PuysicaL Basis orf HEREDITY AND EVOLUTION FROM THE CYTOLOGICAL STANDPOINT JouNn Merte Courter, Professor and Head of the Department of Botany, the University of Chicago Tue Metuop or EVOLUTION HEREDITY AND SEX WiLtiaAmM ERNEST CastLe, Professor of Zodlogy, Har- vard University INHERITANCE IN THE HIGHER PLANTS THE APPLICATION OF BIOLOGICAL PRINCIPLES TO PLANT BREEDING Epwarp Murray East, Assistant Professor of Experi- mental Plant Morphology, Harvard University RECENT ADVANCES AND THE PRESENT STATE OF KNOWL- EDGE CONCERNING THE MODIFICATION OF THE GER- MINAL CONSTITUTION OF ORGANISMS BY EXPERIMENTAL PROCESSES Witt1am LAWRENCE TowWER, Associate Professor of Zodélogy, the University of Chicago Tue INHERITANCE OF PHYSICAL AND MENTAL TRAITS or MAN AND THEIR APPLICATION TO EUGENICS Tue GEOGRAPHY OF MAN IN RELATION TO EUGENICS CuHaRLES BENEDICT DAVENPORT, Station for Experimen- tal Evolution, Carnegie Institution of Washington INDEX vil PAGE 22 39 62 83 113 141 269 289 313 JOHN MERLE COULTER Professor and Head of the Department of Botany The University of Chicago CHAPTER I RECENT DEVELOPMENTS IN HEREDITY AND EVOLUTION: GENERAL INTRODUCTION This series of lectures is intended to present, in outline, the recent development of knowledge in reference to heredity and evolution. These subjects have to do, not only with the most fundamental conceptions of biology, but they have come to be of immense practical importance in animal and plant breeding. From every aspect, therefore, they appeal to all persons intelligent enough to be interested in the progress of knowledge and in human welfare. At the same time, it is recognized that most people are denied the oppor- tunity of knowing the progress that has been made in these subjects, through lack of biological training or lack of time. To them the suggestions of progress have come chiefly through ephemeral and often misleading publications. It is the purpose of this series, therefore, to present this information in such a form that it can be appreciated by those who have no special training in biological work; in short, to interpret the significant results of recent investigations. Before presenting the recent developments in the inves- tigation of heredity and evolution, it is essential to provide a historical background, for nothing is more obvious than that the work of today has evolved gradually from all the work of the past. It should be understood, also, that the subject is so vast in scope and in work that to outline it in a few lectures will require the most rigid selection of material, a selection so rigid that students of the subject will be able to point out glaring omissions. 3 4 Heredity and Eugenics If rigid selection is necessary in presenting the recent work, it must be still more rigid in sketching the historical background, with its enormous literature, stretching through many years. Probably no two biologists would put the same details into the background, but probably all of them would give the background the same general aspect; for it is more of an atmosphere than detail that is needed. It will be an aid to understanding and to memory if this sketch is broken up into distinct topics, if it be understood clearly that there are no such natural lines of cleavage in the subject. I. THE CONCEPTION OF EVOLUTION Those who know of the theory of evolution only in a superficial way, as a thing heard of rather than understood, almost invariably associate it with some man who stands to them as its author. In my own experience, I have encountered a widespread conviction that Darwin is respon- sible for the theory of organic evolution. The fact is that the conception of evolution, both inorganic and organic, is as old as our record of man’s thought, and therefore no one is responsible for it. It is the common property of the human race. However, a sharp distinction must be made between the speculative stage of evolution and the observational stage. The former is imaginative or philosophical, and could not establish evolution as a fact; the latter is scientific, and has e:tablished evolution as a fact. In a real sense, therefore, organic evolution as a definite working principle is compara- tively modern, being but little more than one hundred years old. Recent Developments in Heredity and Evolution 5 2. THE FACT OF EVOLUTION It may be helpful to indicate some of the things that began to open the eyes of thinking men and finally compelled them to accept organic evolution as a fact. 1. The growing proof that the inorganic world had been formed by a process of slow evolution rather than by a series of miraculous catastrophes, compelled the suggestion that the organic world may have developed in the same gradual way by natural processes. 2. The observed intergrading of species, frequently so complete as to make distinct boundaries for species impos- sible, strongly suggested the passing of one into the other. Dr. Asa Gray once remarked that he did not believe there are any species of North American asters, although he had been studying them for twenty years. Of course this was an expression of despair rather than of belief, but it illus- trates the situation. Botanists for a long time emphasized the boundaries of species by preserving in their herbaria what they called ‘‘typical specimens” and discarding the intergrades, so that in turning over the sheets of a herbarium the species looked quite distinct; but any excursion into the field brought trouble. 3. Observations began to multiply showing that plants and animals are often able to respond to changed con- ditions and change their own form or structure. This was called the power of “adaptation,” and it has been a most persistent idea. The fact of change was evident, but its explanation has been outgrown. But taking it as a fact, it was evident that the small changes observed would suggest the possibility of indefinitely extended changes. 6 Heredity and Eugenics 4. More intimate knowledge of the structure of plant and animal bodies revealed what were called “‘ rudimentary ”’ structures, which are quite evidently abandoned structures. This suggested at once that they were functioning structures in the ancestral forms. Even man, and perhaps man most of all, was recognized as being a walking museum of antiquity. 5. Then the life panorama of the geological record began to be unrolled; and it became clear that a fauna and a flora totally unlike that of today existed in the earliest periods; that as one approached the later records, resem- blances began to appear; and that insensibly the fauna and flora of the ancient world merged into those of today. This was historical evidence of tremendous weight in favor of the fact of a gradual organic evolution. 6. Soon what is called embryology began to be studied, and plants and animals were traced, stage by stage, from the egg to the adult form. In the course of this develop- ment resemblances to other forms appeared, which had disappeared when the adult stage was reached. And so the idea developed that here were glimpses of earlier con- nections, and it became formulated in the well-worn state- ment that the history of the individual repeats the history of the race, a theory labeled “recapitulation.” 7. Men’s eyes also began to be opened to the fact that great changes had been wrought in plants by cultivation, and in animals by domestication; so great in many cases that the wild originals could not be recognized with certainty. Later, Darwin called this ‘‘an experiment upon a gigantic scale,” but it was an experiment unconsciously performed. At least it proved that the operations of man could modify plants and animals, and modify them so much that resem- blances to the wild originals would be obscured. Recent Developments in Heredity and Evolution 7 The seven categories of facts thus indicated, and others that might be added to them, will explain why a number of scientific men were so impressed by the idea that organic evolution is a fact, that they thought it important to search for an explanation of the process. 3. THE EXPLANATION OF EVOLUTION To accept organic evolution as a fact, and to explain it as a process are two very different things, and must be kept clearly distinct. The failure to distinguish them has led recently to much confusion in popular statement and belief. For example, the more exact work of recent years has developed a considerable body of criticism against Darwin’s theory of natural selection. To those who thought of the theory of organic evolution as belonging to Darwin, these criticisms seemed to indicate that belief in organic evolution was tottering; when in fact, if any belief was tottering, it was a belief in natural selection as a sufficient explanation of the process of evolution. Darwin’s explana- tion, Lamarck’s explanation, every explanation hitherto proposed, may be found inadequate, and still organic evolution will remain to be explained. It must be remem- bered that the work of biologists has been to explain the fact of organic evolution, not to propose it as an idea; and the destruction of no explanation can weaken the fact. A single address does not permit the mention of all the proposed explanations of organic evolution, but a few domi- nating ones may be selected as illustrations. The selection is made with a full appreciation of the fact that profes- sional biologists may think that others should be included. It is important to distinguish between the two methods of attacking the problem. The earlier method, and the 8 Heredity and Eugenics one that prevailed for nearly one hundred years, was obser- vational. Series of intergrading plants and animals were observed, and by comparing them it was inferred that they represented the series of transformations that had occurred actually in nature. This has remained the only possible method for the paleontologist, and he also has the advantage _ of dealing with series of enormous length. It is to be expected, therefore, that the paleontologist will be impressed most strongly by those explanations of evolution which have been derived from observation and comparison. The later method of attacking the problem, a method that has developed with great rapidity during the last decade, is experimental. Plants and animals are taken in hand and are made to show their possibilities. It should be kept in mind that the problem is to explain how one species can produce another. The study of organic evolution deals only with the succession of forms, with the production of new forms by previously existing ones. It has nothing to say concerning origins. How the numerous series of living forms may have originated is certainly beyond the reach of biological science as yet. When one goes beyond the observed changes, and tries to trace the successions back to their source, he is in a region of specu- lation, and outside the boundaries of science. One may stand beside a great stream and discover that its waters are moving; he also recognizes the direction of the move- ment; but he can know nothing of the distant sources of the stream, for he sees only a very small section. So the scientific recognition of organic evolution simply observes the movement and its direction. The sources are far too distant for observation, and the possibilities are too numerous for profitable speculation. It is evident that Recent Developments in Heredity and Evolution 9 people in general are more interested in speculation than in plain facts; and they are so interested in such speculations as the origin of life and the origin of man that they come to believe that these speculations belong to the scientific study of organic evolution. But we are simply collecting the facts of change and trying to discover the causes and processes of change in the plants and animals that can come under our observation. We can thus discover laws of evolution, just as we discover the law of gravitation, by observing them in operation. Of course the ultimate questions continually suggest themselves, but it must not be thought that any proposed answers to them are a part of biological science. 1. Environment.—The first attempt at what might be called a scientific explanation of organic evolution, because based upon observation, was that it is caused by changes in environment. This explanation began to take definite form during the last decade of the eighteenth century, in the writings of such observers as Erasmus Darwin of England, St. Hilaire of France, and Goethe of Germany. Environ- ment is a term quite variable in its biological application, but we do not need to discuss it in this connection. These older observers saw changes occurring in plants and animals (especially the latter), in response to changes in seasons, in exposure, in climate, etc.; and their picture of the process of evolution was that plants and animals are plastic organ- isms that are being molded by their environment. The environment and the molding were not analyzed, but thought of in a very superficial sense; so that it was not long before it was recognized that the changes thus induced are too superficial and ephemeral to furnish an adequate explanation of evolution. But it must not be forgotten 10 Heredily and Eugenics that environment, even in its superficial sense, is a very real factor, and has played its part in every evolutionary theory since. 2. Use and disuse.—In the early part of the nineteenth century, the first substantial explanation of organic evolu- tion was proposed. Its author was Lamarck, and the theory has become styled Lamarckism or Lamarckianism, but its author called it ‘‘appetency,’’ or the doctrine of desires. It is more intelligible to the uninformed as the effect of use and disuse. This explanation has been a conspicuous part of evolutionary doctrine ever since, and in modified form is known today as neo-Lamarckianism. The conception is simple enough and has a basis of facts. It is well known, for example, that use develops a muscle, and that disuse deteriorates it, a deterioration that may reach as far as inability to function. If this effect of use and disuse be applicable to all organs and regions of the body, and certain conditions of living were to change, demanding the use of structures that had not been called upon to do so much service before, and also excusing from such constant service structures that had been very active before, one might imagine changes taking place in the greater development of certain structures and the less development of others. In other words, change in the environment means change in the demands on the structures of plants and animals, and these demands are met by the active exertion of the organism. A well-known illustration used by Lamarck will serve our purpose. A grazing animal, with an ordinary neck, is placed in conditions that demand feeding upon the foliage of trees. The continuous use of the neck in stretching would cause it to increase somewhat in length. This slight increase in length would be transmitted to the next Recent Developments in Heredity and Evolution II generation, which in turn would add to it, until a number of generations would succeed in developing the exaggerated neck of the giraffe. This illustration makes clear the factors relied upon by Lamarck, namely, the effect of use demanded by changed conditions, and the transmission of the changes from parent to offspring. His own name, “appetency,” sought to ex- press the idea of striving to satisfy a desire; but, as might have been expected, it was not understood by most of the people of his day, and lent itself admirably to all sorts of caricature. The changes in structure brought about during the life of an individual are spoken of as “acquired characters,”’ and Lamarck’s explanation of the evolutionary process would be impossible if acquired characters are not trans- mitted from parent to offspring. The present consensus of opinion seems to be that such acquired characters as Lamarck had in mind are not transmissible; but the whole subject of the transmission of acquired characters is more a matter of definition than anything else. 3. Natural selection—It was the explanation offered by Charles Darwin, however, that proved to be the most epoch-making theory in the history of biological science. He called it ‘‘natural selection,” and it has been a domi- nating conception for fifty years. With the Darwin centennial celebrations only two years old, and with the flood of literature that accompanied and followed them, no one interested in the subject of evolution can be ignorant of the meaning of natural selection, or of the revolution in thought and method brought about by its presentation in Darwin’s Origin of Species. While Lamarck’s conception was based upon extensive observation, and therefore was 12 Heredity and Eugenics reached in a thoroughly scientific way, Darwin’s conception was based upon an amount and range of observation hitherto unapproached; so that if Lamarck’s approach was scien- tific, Darwin’s was still more scientific. In fact, Darwin’s announcement came at a psychological moment, which enormously reinforced his message; and this is not detract- ing in the least from the power and beauty of its presenta- tion. Whether Darwin’s explanation stands or falls, his supreme contribution must be regarded as the introduction of a point of view and a method of attack that not only ushered in modern biology, but also revolutionized thought in general. Natural selection is too familiar to need extended expla- nation. The ratio of increase of organisms, leading to over-production and a struggle for existence, resulting in the survival of the fittest, is a series of exceedingly familiar phrases, not all of which should be attributed to Darwin. That plants and animals can be led along in any desired direction was proved by experimental evidence obtained from the operations of plant and animal breeders; and since this guidance of plants and animals by man was by means of selection, it was most appropriate to call the guidance by nature ‘‘natural selection.”’ The most significant fact connected with this theory remains to be mentioned, and that is the fact of variation. Nothing is more clear than that any machinery of evolution must depend upon this fact. Darwin greatly enlarged the horizon of our knowledge in reference to variation. It is variation that gives rise to individuality among plants and animals, so that no two plants or animals are exactly alike. We have accustomed ourselves to individuality among human beings, for we have been trained to note the dis- Recent Developments in Heredity and Evolution 13 tinguishing marks. But this same individuality is no less true for all animals and plants. In heredity, therefore, there is transmitted not only a likeness to the parent, but also an unlikeness, and this unlikeness constitutes indi- viduality, a certain amount of variation from the parent. Darwin’s conception was that nature selects from among these varying individuals; that the means of selec- tion is the competition that results from over-production; that the better adapted individuals would naturally be selected for survival; that their better adaptations, which mean their individual peculiarities, would be transmitted to their offspring; and that such selection, continued genera- tion after generation, would so emphasize and increase the favored variations that the old species boundary would be crossed and a new species established. In other words, small variations would be built up into larger ones, and presently they would become too large to be included within the boundary of the old species. Of course, objections have been raised to the theory of natural selection as an adequate explanation of the origin of species. There can be no doubt but that there is selec- tion in nature, in the sense that not all the forms produced survive; but many believe that this cannot change forms enough to be regarded as new species; that any selection thus made cannot be on the basis of any “life-and-death”’ advantage of structure that one individual has over another; and that the variations thus used are only the so-called “fluctuating variations’? which have nothing definite in them as to direction or amount. 4. Mutation—We come now to the work of the last decade, which is characterized by the rapid development of the study of evolution by using experimental methods. 14 Heredity and Eugenics Perhaps the most influential work to enforce the experi- mental method was that of DeVries, in developing his theory of mutation. His great contribution, therefore, must not be regarded as offering mutation as an explana- tion of the origin of species, for that explanation may not stand, but as establishing, or at least powerfully helping to establish, the study of evolution upon an experimental basis. The mutation theory needs no extended explanation, for the current literature of organic evolution is full of it. The long series of cultures of Oenothera, under rigid control and in large numbers, are familiar. The appearance, in relatively very small numbers, of widely different indi- viduals, which “‘came true”’ in subsequent generations, led to the inference that new species were appearing under observation, suddenly produced by the parent form, fully equipped as species, without any intermediate stages or any building up by selection. It should be noted that this does not banish natural selection as a factor in evolution, but assigns to it a new role, which is not to produce species, but to select among those already produced. The study of mutations is one of the vigorous phases of experimental work today, and some of the results will be presented in the subsequent chapters. Objections to the theory have developed, as must be the case in all theories. There are questions as to the extent of mutation as a process going on among plants and animals; as to its reliability in producing species; as to whether mutants are really new forms, or only old ones derived from a splitting hybrid parent. It is such questions, and others like them, that experimental work today is trying to answer. 5. Orthogenesis.—The barest kind of evolutionary back- ground would be inadequate without a mention of ortho- Recent Developments in Heredity and Evolution 15 genesis. The variations utilized in the preceding explana- tions, both the smaller ones used by natural selection and the larger ones used by mutation, occur in every direction from the parent form, the successful direction being deter- mined by natural selection. This has been called indeter- minate variation. In tracing the evolution of great groups, however, it becomes clear that the most important varia- tions occur in certain definite directions, which have been maintained persistently throughout all possible changes of condition. For example, the history of such a group as gymnosperms shows a tendency to vary in certain definite directions that has persisted from the early Paleozoic to the present time. In other words, there is much to indicate that while variation may be indeterminate, there are also certain definite lines that persist. The origin of new forms, whether by natural selection or mutation or neither, as the result of a persistent determinate variation, is called ortho- genesis. It certainly removes one of the greatest difficul- ties in the way of natural selection, and that is the beginning and development of a structure that can be of advantage only when it is completed. It satisfies also the many known cases of excessive development in certain directions, a development that may be not only disadvantageous, but even destructive. Even if determinate variation is accepted as a fact, however, what determines the persistent variation ? The answer to this question has resulted in many variations of the theory of orthogenesis. It should be noted that natural selection, mutation, and orthogenesis are not mutually destructive. They all deal with variations, and may all be operative in producing new forms. Natural selection deals with small variations which are in every direction; mutation with large variations which 10 Heredity and Eugenics are in every direction; and orthogenesis with those small or large and relatively few variations which for some reason persist and increase from generation to generation and carry forward the group as a whole. 4. BIOMETRY When the idea of natural selection became dominant, and new species were believed to have arisen by the accumu- lation of small variations, which seemed to be indefinite and fluctuating, a statistical method of attack began to be developed, a method that has been named biometry. The most conspicuous names that one meets in the literature of biometry are Galton, the English pioneer in the exploitation of the method; and Pearson, who has been largely instru- mental in carrying it forward into its more advanced mathematical stage. It is a method that deals with groups or populations, rather than with individuals, and its results present the averages of variations. It is evident that an average obtained from the measurements of a given character in a series of individuals will depend upon the individuals selected for measurement. Therefore, biometry demands to an unusual degree the elimination of preconceived opin- ions and the exercise of great judgment. It has become so special and intricate a method that it can be followed, with full understanding, only by those with special training; so that any adequate illustration of it will be left to such of the subsequent addresses as may have occasion to apply it. A word can be said, however, concerning its use as an instrument in the study of the processes of evolution. Selecting any character or group of characters that are to be measured or counted, and using a wisely selected range of Recent Developments in Heredity and Evolution 17 material, biometry reveals the prevailing tendency, in reference to these variations, in a group of individuals representing a species, a tendency that could not be recog- nized by the study of a single individual. Applying this method to successive generations from this group of indi- viduals, under experimental control, it can be discovered whether the prevailing tendency in the expression of these variations remains the same, continuing the species as before; or shifts, modifying the species as a whole; or splits up, giving rise to a second marked tendency, that may mean presently two distinct species. It is evident that such data can be very suggestive, but that their limitations must be recognized. They are data concerning successive populations, and show the average result of individual variation as expressed in a population. In other words, they present in concrete and somewhat definite form the problems of variation and inheritance that must be solved. 5. HEREDITY It must have become evident, during the preceding sketch of representative theories of evolution, that the fundamental factor in the process is variation, and that the essential and inevitable question behind all of these explana- tions is the origin of variation. This brings us at once to the problem of heredity, with its supposed processes for transmitting what we call ‘“‘characters” from parent to offspring. How is variation secured in this transmission ? The earlier observers simply accepted variation as a fact, and made no serious attempt to explain it. The first attack upon this problem was the accumulation of data in reference to the facts of heredity. To accumulate 18 Heredity and Eugenics these facts in such numbers as to make any generaliza- tion worthy, demands the culture through many generations, under most rigid control, of the largest possible number of plants and animals. This means long periods of time, great patience, and many investigators. The number of investi- gators is multiplying, the range of material is increasing, and the period covered by some of the work has now been sufficient to justify some presentation of the results. Probably the most conspicuous working hypothesis today, in connection with the collection and interpreta- tion of the data of heredity, is the one called ‘‘ Mendel’s law.”’ This is to be made a special theme in the course of this series, but a brief statement in reference to it will help the background and may prepare the way for the later discussion. This Austrian monk, who worked in _ his monastery garden during the middle of the last century, left on record what is called a law of heredity. This record was lost, so far as its influence was concerned, until ten or fifteen years ago, when the modern movement in experimental evolution began to be vigorous. Now the Mendelians constitute a conspicuous biological cult, and Mendelism has extended from its simple original state- ment into a speculative philosophy, with conceptions of unit-characters, dominance, ratios, etc., that the untrained cannot follow. The fundamental conception is simple enough. If two different species are crossed, the result is a hybrid which combines certain characters of both parents. When this hybrid propagates, the progeny splits up into three sets: one set resembling the hybrid parent; and the two other sets resembling the parent forms that entered into the hybrid. Mendel’s law is a statement of the definite ratio Recent Developments in Heredity and Evolution 19 expressed by these three groups of forms derived from a splitting hybrid. This means that in a series of genera- tions initiated by a hybrid, approximately one-half of the individuals of each generation will represent the hybrid mixture, one-fourth of the individuals will represent one of the pure forms that entered into the hybrid, and the remaining fourth will represent the other pure form. It should be understood that the use of hybrids in such experimental work is simply a device to secure easy recog- nition of the contributions of each parent to the progeny. For example, if red and yellow races of corn are crossed, it is very simple to recognize the color contribution of each parent to the hybrid progeny, when it would be impossible to separate the contribution of two yellow parents. The inference is, that what is true of hybrids is true of forms produced in the ordinary way, so that laws of heredity obtained from a study of hybrids may be regarded as laws of heredity in general. In one sense, every union of parent forms is hybridizing, for each parent has its own individuality. One of the more subtle problems that has arisen in connection with such investigations is the problem of sex determination. In all organisms with sex differentiation, progeny is produced by the fusion of male and female sexual cells, and this progeny develops as distinct male and female individuals. It is one thing to determine the general structure of an organism by some law of heredity, but a very different thing to determine why any individual thus produced is sometimes male and sometimes female. The work of today is not resting content with the patient collection of the facts of heredity, with determining ratios as expressions of laws, and with the end results of processes 20 Heredity and Eugenics initiated under experimental control. There is keen search- ing for cytological evidence; and the structure and behavior of the sexual cells, through the whole process of their forma- tion, during the act of fusion, and in the initiating activity of the fertilized egg, are being subjected to all the scrutiny that a developing technique can devise to discover the mechanism of heredity. Moreover, cytological evidence is being searched for not only to discover a possible physical basis for heredity, which would mean actual material machinery, but also to discover the possible relations of chemical and physical factors to this most fundamental and obscure process. It is believed that it must be a response, in terms of chemistry and physics, by a living material substance. 6. PRACTICAL APPLICATIONS In a university atmosphere, the chief interest is probably focused upon the attempt to reveal one of the so-called “mysteries” of life, those mysteries which always invite one to uncover them; but there is another aspect of these problems worth considering. The experimental work that has been done in the study of heredity and evolution has had a very important bearing upon the practical handling of plants and animals, including the human animal. The applica- tions have been made to plants most extensively, and methods of plant breeding have been revolutionized. The recognition that commercially ‘‘pure seed” is an extensive mixture of different types or strains, has led to their separation, and has changed the clumsy and _ inefficient method of mass culture to the definite and exact method of pedigree culture. As a consequence, the number of forms made available for culture has been multiplied enormously, Recent Developments in Heredity and Evolution 21 having simply been discovered and pedigreed, without the labor of continuous selection year after year, and without the old inconstancy in the result. To reduce labor, to multiply cultural forms, and to obtain constant results in plant breeding are results of very large importance to the material side of human welfare. Pedigree culture has not only multiplied available forms, but it has begun to be used most effectively in combating drought and disease, the most dangerous enemies of cultivated plants. Drought- resistant races are being developed from pedigreed stock, and immunity to different diseases has been found to be a transmissible character. When it is remembered that drought-resistance not only insures crops over areas now cultivated, but also secures an enormous extension of area that can be cultivated; and that the annual losses from plant diseases represent an enormous financial total; it will be appreciated that the study of heredity and evolution, with purely scientific purpose, incidentally has been enor- mously profitable on the practical side. I have sketched a background that will permit those who follow to put their work in its setting without much loss of time. The interest lies chiefly in the foreground that they will develop, for it will represent the work of today. CHAPTER II THE PHYSICAL BASIS OF HEREDITY AND EVOLUTION FROM THE CYTOLOGICAL STANDPOINT Heredity involves not only the transmission of similarity in structure, but also the transmission of dissimilarity. Likeness means close relation to the parent forms; unlike- ness means individuality. It is not generally appreciated that individuality expresses itself just as certainly among plants and animals as among human beings. We have learned to recognize the marks of individuality among human beings through long acquaintance, but we should realize also that no two plants or animals are exactly alike. It is this individuality that is called variation, and variation is the basis of evolution. The phenomena of heredity established as facts by series of cultures under rigid control, however, must be recognized as the end results, between which and the act of fertilization there extends a series of unknown processes, with which as yet only scientific imagi- nation deals. The purpose of this chapter is to inquire whether there is any physical basis for the transmission of like and unlike characters, in the same sense that protoplasm was long ago called ‘‘the physical basis of life.’ This phrase only means that protoplasm is the material substance in which the phenomena of life are manifested. Is there any substance or structure by means of which the phenomena of heredity manifest themselves ? The answer to this question would be given more appro- priately by some biologist who has made it the special 22 Physical Basis of Heredity and Evolution 23 subject of his investigations. This chapter, therefore, must be regarded simply as the presentation of a teacher, to explain a subject that belongs logically in the series. Another restriction is that this presentation deals chiefly with structures that are visible by means of laboratory technique, and not with the results of experiment upon these structures. A final restriction is that the statements deal with plants, a limitation necessary to the writer, and offset by the fact that the corresponding facts in animals will be stated in one of the later chapters. Some conception of what is meant by the power of repro- duction will be useful. Among the simplest plants, every cell has this power; in fact, some plants are so simple that the adult body consists of a single cell. As plant bodies came to be made up of numerous cells, some of them lost the power of reproduction; and as the body became increas- ingly complex, the number of cells retaining the power of reproduction became relatively smaller. This means that in the complex plant body, the relatively few reproductive cells are not so much “‘cells set apart for this special func- tion,” as cells that have not lost this primary power. The specialized cells are not those that reproduce, but those that cannot. The loss of reproductive power is usually not complete, for most cells can reproduce their own kind, even if they cannot reproduce the whole body. This leads to a consideration of what is included in full reproductive power. Without including confusing details, it may be said that such reproduction as one has in mind in connection with heredity involves four general things. First, there is cell multiplication, the fertilized egg initiating a series of cell divisions that may result in a multitude of cells. It is evident, however, that a complex plant body is 24 Heredity and Eugenics more than a multitude of similar cells. In the second place, there is cell differentiation, groups of cells becoming different, so that the various tissues, with their special functions, are developed. In the third place, tissues must be organized together into the structures called organs. In the last place, the organs must be combined in making that total organization called the individual. It is this far-reaching directive influence that is the most baffling fact in connection with heredity. To obtain any impression of the supposed machinery of heredity, it is necessary to know something of the structure of a living cell. So far as material goes, such a cell is an individualized mass of protoplasm. This protoplasm is organized into a body known conveniently as the proto- plast, which is the living body of the cell. In plants, the protoplast usually constructs a cellulose wall about itself, which has given rise to the impression that a cell is a walled chamber containing protoplasm. In the plant cells which have to do directly with heredity, however, namely the reproductive cells, the cellulose wall is not formed, and they are naked protoplasts. The protoplast is exceedingly complex, as cytologists well know, and includes a variety of organs. Conspicuous among these protoplasmic organs is the nucleus, which is a more or less spherical body and usually sharply limited from the rest of the protoplast, in which it lies imbedded (Fig. 1). The remainder of the protoplasmic material enters into the structure of the cytoplasm, another organ or region of the protoplast constantly associated with the nucleus. Every living cell contains a nucleus and cytoplasm, and in addition there may be other protoplasmic organs (Fig. 1), but the two mentioned are those that belong to this discussion. Physical Basis of Heredity and Evolution 25 As in the case of all organs, the cytoplasm and the nucleus are associated with special functions. This does not mean that each does a certain thing and nothing else, but that each is conspicuous in connection with a certain kind of work. Especially unsafe is it to ascribe certain definite functions to these organs of the protoplast, because pro- toplasm itself is so little understood. In any event, the cytoplasm seems to be conspicuously associated with the metabolic activi- ties of the cell; and it is certain that the nucleus is conspicuously associated with cell division. When division occurs, and one cell gives rise to two cells, this process almost invari- ably begins with the nu- cleus, which may thus be said to initiate cell division. It must be understood clearly that we are speak- ing of visible changes in structure, behind which and Fic. 1.—Cells from a moss leaf: each of the complete cells shows the well- defined nucleus; since the leaf is green, there are also numerous green protoplas- mic organs (chloroplasts); the remaining cranular-looking ground substance is the cytoplasm. accompanying which there are certainly numerous invisible physical and chemical changes. It is evident that the problem of heredity is involved in the process of cell division, for through this process the old cell transmits whatever determines the characters of 20 Heredity and Eugenics the two new cells. In ordinary cell division, the imme- diate transmission seems to be a similar structure, for the new cells resemble the old one in all recognizable features. In the differentiation of cells, however, certain cell divisions involve the transmission of unlike characters. This relation of the nucleus to cell division, and of cell division to heredity, has focused the attention of cytolo- gists upon the structure and behavior of the nucleus. No structure of plants and animals has received such detailed and persistent investigation as has the nucleus, and much of the advance in technique associated with the use of the microscope has been stimulated by the necessity of learning more about the nucleus. If the nucleus is the conspicuous structure associated with cell division, the suggestion is natural that it is the material structure associated with heredity. But the nucleus is a complex, and most conspicuous in its structure is a substance called chromatin. In the ordinary nucleus it appears as a network of denser material, which has received its name from the fact that it takes stains easily, being the most stainable substance in the nucleus (Fig. 2,a). If there is any definite material that deserves to be called the physical basis of heredity, it is probably chro- matin, which of course is a protoplasmic substance. This belief is largely based upon the behavior of chromatin during cell division. In preparation for division, the chromatin network becomes an evident continuous band, which resembles a tangled skein (Fig. 2, 6). This band shortens and corre- spondingly thickens, and finally breaks up into a definite number of units, called chromosomes (Fig. 2, c). These chromosomes are thus chromatin individually organized, Physical Basis of Heredity and Evolution 27 in the same sense that protoplasts are protoplasm indi- vidually organized, and they are thought to retain their individual identity through all the apparent fusions into bands and network. It is the chromosome, therefore, consisting of the material chromatin, that is regarded as the organized carrier of transmissible characters; hence the behavior of the chromosomes in cell division becomes a Fic. 2.—Diagram of stages in nuclear division.—After Lock matter of first importance in considering the machinery of heredity. One of the important facts to note is the definite number of chromosomes. Each kind of plant and animal has its own number. For example, in certain plants the number is 6; in others it may be considerably more than too; and of course the intermediate numbers are well represented. Differences in the number of chromosomes may occur 28 Heredity and Eugenics among very closely related plants, or the number may be constant throughout a great group. [or example, in the gymnosperms, so far as known, the number is almost con- stantly 12. This fact has given rise to the suggestion that the number of chromosomes, as well as their quality, may be a factor in heredity, but too much stress must not be laid upon it as yet. While the chromosomes are becoming separate, a spindle of fibers is formed about the nucleus, and the chromosomes become attached to the fibers, finally being arranged about the equator of the spindle (Fig. 2, d). In this position, each chromosome splits longitudinally (Fig. 2, e), and the two halves, by the shortening of attached fibers, are drawn toward the opposite poles of the spindle (Fig. 2, f), the old chromosome thus being represented at each pole by a half-chromosome. The half-chromosomes at each pole enter into the organization of a new nucleus (I'ig. 2, g), wall material is deposited in the plane of the equator of the spindle and, extending through the cytoplasm, cuts the old cell into two new cells, each with its nucleus (Fig. 2, i). It is evident that each new nucleus has the same number of chromosomes as the old one, and that each chromosome of the new nuclei represents in material a chromosome of the parent nucleus. This detailed and precise process in the behavior of chromosomes, insuring the transmission from one cell to its progeny cells of the identical material contained in every chromosome, is a strong argument in favor of regard- ing the chromosome as the carrier of hereditary qualities. The kind of reproduction with which the problems of heredity are concerned chiefly, however, is that which involves the fusion of sexual cells. There are three distinct Physical Basis of Heredity and Evolution 20 methods of reproduction recognized among plants. The most primitive method is vegetative multiplication, ordinary vegetative cells producing new plants. In the transmission of hereditary qualities this involves simply a series of such cell divisions as has been described above. Later in the evolution of plants, the power of reproduction was dis- played chiefly by spores, which at first were only certain protoplasts that escaped from the incasing wall. In most cases, before escape the protoplast divides, so that there issue from the old cell two or more naked protoplasts or spores. Since the early spores belonged to water plants, they had swimming append- ages (cilia), and were called swim- ming spores or zodspores (Fig. 3, a and 6). Any one of these swim- zy , Tic. 3.—A portion of a fila- ming spores, under appropriate ment of agreen alga (Ulothrix): conditions, can produce a new in- 4; 200spores in mother cell; 6, ss ° : an escaped zodspore; c, ga- dividual. This method of multi- | ites most of which have plying individuals remains the most — escaped from the mother cell; effective method among plants. - re pening and Tusings Vegetable multiplication and 77" ~ reproduction by spores are both sexless methods, and it is quite evident that the introduction of the sexual method is more significant than merely a third method of reproduction. It is demonstrable among plants that the sexual cells (gametes) were derived from the swimming spores (zodspores). In certain plants, if a protoplast divides and gives rise to 20 Heredity and Eugenics few and comparatively large protoplasts, they are swim- ming spores (Fig. 3, a@ and 0), and each can reproduce. I the divisions continue, however, and result in numerous and comparatively small protoplasts, they are unable to reproduce (Fig. 3, c). However, if they come together in pairs and fuse (Fig. 3, d), thus making one cell (protoplast) out of two (Fig. 3, e), the new cell can reproduce. This fusion is the sexual act, and the fusing cells are the sexual cells (gametes). It is of interest to note that this first appearance of sex is quite disconnected with the multipii- cation of individuals. Individuals are multiplied through- out the growing season by the spores. Toward the close of the season, the gametes begin to appear, and the fusion cells (sygotes) formed by their pairing develop heavy walls that protect them through the unfavorable season (as the winter). All the other structures of the plant perish, and it exists through the winter only in the form of zygotes. At the beginning of the next season, the zygotes produce new plants, and these are multiplied by spores. The service rendered by the sex act in this case, therefore, is to produce a protected cell, which can carry the plant through an unfavorable period; in short, the service is protection rather than multiplication. The next advance in the evolution of sex was its differ- entiation. The gametes at first are similar in appearance and in behavior, but it must be recognized that this optical test would be unable to detect any differences in quality. It is upon the basis of appearance that such gametes are said to be unisexual, which only means that they cannot be distinguished as male and female. A series can be arranged to illustrate a gradual differentiation of gametes into two forms, unlike in appearance and in behavior. In Physical Basis of Heredity and Evolution 31 one case, the gamete becomes larger and larger, its power of movement diminishing at the same time, until at last it becomes a very large and entirely passive cell. In the other case, the gamete retains its small size and activity. These two very dissimilar gametes are the egg and the sperm, easily distinguishable female and male cells (Fig. 4). The essential difference thus brought about is the great in- Fic. 4.—A single large egg and numerous small sperms of rockweed (Fucus) crease in the bulk of the gamete that becomes the egg, but the constant feature, which is not changed, is the chro- matin, the increase in bulk being due to an increase of cytoplasm. This constancy of the chromatin, coupled with the known fact that the two gametes contribute alike to their progeny, indicates that the chromatin is the essential material in heredity. Since both cytoplasm and nuclei are involved in the sexual fusion, it may be claimed that the iS} Heredity and Eugenics Ww cytoplasm is as essential to the process as the nucleus; but in certain plants, notably Lilium, it has been demonstrated that when fusion occurs there is no cytoplasm whatsoever investing the male nucleus. It seems safe to conclude, therefore, that the nucleus contains the material essential to the phenomena of heredity; and if so, chromatin must be the material, and the chromosomes its visible organized units. A very simple case will serve to illustrate the results of the sexual fusion upon the chromosome situation. Imagine the fusion of an egg and a 4 sperm, each of whose nuclei contains two chromosomes (+) LB s (Fig. §,z and 2), ‘The nu- 1 ae cleus of the fertilized egg : NO g would contain four chromo- somes (Fig. 5, 3), two of : oe te them maternal (contributed - by the egg), and two of Fic. 5.—Diagram illustrating result them paternal (contributed of sexual fusion: zr, sperm containing two chromosomes (4A); 2, egg containing by the sperm). Suppose two chromosomes (BB); 3, fertilized egg that two of the four domi- containing four chromosomes (two Pa- nate in determining the ternal and two maternal); 4, domination een of paternal chromosomes; 5 and 6,domi- structure of the new indi- nation of one paternal and one maternal vidual to be developed from chromosome; 7, domination of maternal per - Sane arias the fertilized egg. It will be seen that there are four possible pairs: (1) the paternal pair (Fig. 5, 4), in which case the new individuals would resemble the male parent; (2) the maternal pair (Fig. 5, 7), in which case the resem- blance would be to the female parent; (3) two pairs (Fig. 5, 5 and 6), each consisting of a dominant male and a dominant Physical Basis of Heredity and Evolution 3 WwW female chromosome, in which case the new individual would be a mixture, resembling both parents. Expressing the chances in the form of a ratio, they could be represented as 1:2:1. This is a simple expression of Mendel’s law, defined in the preceding chapter. The formulation of Mendel’s law was based upon the observed facts of heredity; and this chromosome situation supplies for it a cytological basis. It must be understood clearly that so simple an illus- tration does not represent the actual facts, for chromosomes are usually more numerous, and eggs and sperms already contain a mixture of paternal and maternal chromosomes derived from the preceding generations. It does illustrate, however, the mixture of hereditary qualities by the sexual fusion, the domination of certain of these qualities, and the chances of resemblances in the progeny. It should be understood also that the structural resemblance to the maternal form or to the paternal form does not include sex determination. For example, the new individual which resembles the maternal form may prove to be either male or female, and vice versa. It is evidently impossible that the chromosomes con- tinue to be doubled at each generation, without any reverse process of reduction. The two cardinal points in every life- history, therefore, are fertilization, by means of which the chromosomes are doubled, and reduction, by means of which the doubled number is halved. The reduction pro- cess occurs at different stages in the life-history in different organisms. Among animals, it occurs in connection with the formation of the eggs and sperms; and therefore reduc- tion and fertilization are practically consecutive events. In most plants, however, the two processes are farther apart, separated from one another by two distinct indi- 34 Heredity and Eugenics viduals, one characterized by the reduced number of chro- mosomes and bearing the sex organs (hence called the gametophyte), the other characterized by the doubled num- ber of chromosomes and producing spores (hence called the sporophyte). In general, it may be said that among animals reduction occurs in connection with gamete forma- tion, while among plants it occurs in connection with spore formation. To use the simpler illustration of animals, it is evident that the cells making up the body of an individual contain the doubled number of chromosomes (Fig. 6, 1), derived from the fertilized egg from which the body developed. _ Fic. 6.—Diagram illustrat- When the sexual cells (eggs or ing result of reduction: 1, ‘ ‘ wk ordinary céllof the body cone SPeTms) of this imdividual are taining doubled number of formed, the reduction occurs, and “onions 2-3, the eggs or sperms contain the re number, and illustrating the ducednumber. How the maternal possibilities in the distribu- and paternal chromosomes are dis- tion of paternal (4A) and A : : ri iutishual (BBY clitemoceiies tributed in this reduction may not be clear, but it is evident that either eggs or sperms may contain chromosomes derived from either line of descent (Fig. 6). In other words, it is not inconceivable to think of an egg containing only chro- mosomes derived from the paternal side, or of a sperm con- taining only chromosomes derived from the maternal side, or most likely of both eggs and sperms containing chromo- somes derived from both sides (Fig. 6, 2-5). These considerations indicate how every sexual fusion results in a complex of possibilities, and the study of heredity Physical Basis of Heredity and Evolution 35 is to discover whether these possibilities can be formulated into a law. In this connection, the phenomenon of parthenogenesis should be referred to. By definition, it means the pro- duction of a new individual by an unfertilized egg, and it is a very common phenomenon among plants. When it is remembered that ordinary cells can produce new individ- uals by vegetative multiplication, and that spores can reproduce, it should not be thought strange that so well- nourished a cell as the egg can do the same thing. Among the higher plants, however, in which the whole mechanism has been worked out in greater detail, there is a significant fact connected with the cases of parthenogenesis. In every case investigated, the reduction in connection with spore formation has not occurred, and therefore the unfertilized egg contains the doubled number of chromosomes, just as though it had been fertilized. In parthenogenesis, therefore, the indications are that the fact to be explained is not reproduc- tion by an unfertilized egg, but the failure of reduction. The whole history of sexual reproduction among plants indicates that its primary significance is not reproduction, for probably many more individuals are produced by vege- tative multiplication and by spores than by the sex act. This would mean that the sexual method is chiefly con- cerned with other results, which are secured in connection with reproduction. These results seem to be the continual securing of new combinations, and new combinations cer- tainly make for evolutionary progress. WILLIAM ERNEST CASTLE Professor of Zodlogy, Harvard University CHAPTER III THE METHOD OF EVOLUTION" No one today doubts the reality of evolution, at least no one does who has had practical experience in animal or plant breeding, and who has seen new forms of life come into being under his own observation and guidance. But the method of evolution is still in doubt. It is known in general, that like begets like, but that occasionally it begets unlike, and this may become a new race. As to how the new race is begotten we have not got much beyond Darwin; indeed many of us have not got so far. For Darwin recog- nized two distinct ways in which new races may arise, but many biologists today insist that there is only one way, the way in which Minerva was begotten, who ‘sprang full-fledged from the head of Jove.”” The modern name for this method of origin is mutation, and its advocates, like the ‘‘followers of the prophet,” insist that there is no other. Darwin was well aware that new races may arise in this way, particularly under domestication, as in the case of the Ancon ram and Niata cattle; but he believed that a far commoner and more important method, particularly among wild species, consists in a slow and gradual modi- fication of the race, constantly in one direction, as under the ever-growing power of a hydraulic press, until the ‘In these two chapters, especially in the second (chap. iv), material has been drawn freely from the writer’s book on Heredity in Relation to Evolution and Animal Breeding (D. Appleton & Co., New York), for which he has the kind permission of the publishers. He also wishes to acknowledge aid given by the Carnegie Institution to the investigations herein described. 39 40 Heredity and Eugenics descendants become so different from their progenitors that man assigns them to distinct races. Now I am inclined to think that Darwin was on the whole nearer the truth than the mutationists. They have perceived a half-truth and perceived it more clearly than did Darwin, but in scrutinizing this they have lost sight of the larger picture which he saw. Darwin saw that new races arise in two ways, and I shall attempt to show that he was right. First let us discuss the Minerva-like method of evolu- tion, the birth of new races in a day, a method of great theoretical interest and practical importance. What is known about this method of evolution is commonly called Mendelism, after Gregor Mendel, an Austrian monk of the last century. Mendel was a school teacher who studied as well as taught, and fortunately for us he studied from the book of nature more than from other books. He thought clearly about the things he saw, but wrote little. Indeed we wish that he had written more, but perhaps if he had done so he would have thought less well. Like most profound thinkers, he was in advance of his day, so that when he spoke, the “wise men”’ of the time failed to under- stand him. The ‘wise man” to whom Mendel hoped to make his ideas plain was the great German botanist, Karl Naegeli, to whom Mendel wrote a number of letters about his studies of plant hybrids. Naegeli failed to grasp the important point in Mendel’s work, and the letters were for- gotten until Mendel’s fame had become world-wide. Then they were hunted up and published. Naegeli’s failure to understand Mendel is after all not surprising; Mendel’s thinking was in advance of his time. Several biological principles now considered commonplaces were then un- The Method of Evolution 41 known. When these had been established, Mendel’s law was independently rediscovered long after his death. All honor to the rediscoverers, DeVries, Correns, and Tschermak, that, honoring the all-but-forgotten monk, they called the new-found law Mendel’s, rather than their own! In Mendel’s time little was known about the nature of the reproductive bodies from which new individuals arise, or of how these bodies are produced, or how they differ from the organisms which produce them. These points must be considered briefly. An old but ever-recurring question in regard to heredity is this: Does one generation inherit any part of the experience of the previous generation ? In other words, is a character acquired by one generation inherited by the next ? This question, first raised in concrete form by Weismann, has been discussed pro and con for many years, but the con- sensus of scientific opinion at the present time favors Weis- mann’s idea that acquired characters are not inherited. In forming a judgment on this question, one fundamental fact should be borne in mind, that in the higher animals body plasm and germ plasm are distinct; that is, the body is distinct from the reproductive cells which it contains, and out of which the next generation is produced. Influences which affect the body have no necessary influence on the germ cells. Weismann some years ago demonstrated this experi- mentally for mutilations of the body. When the tails of mice were cut off generation after generation, it was found that young of the mutilated parents had tails as long as other mice. More distinct evidence of the independence of germ plasm and body is furnished by an experiment recently performed by Dr. Phillips and myself. 42 Heredity and Eugenics A young female albino guinea-pig approaching sexual maturity was deprived of her ovaries, and into her body was introduced the living ovary of a freshly killed black guinea-pig, about three weeks old (Figs. 7 and 8). She was later mated with an albino guinea-pig (Fig. 9). By him she bore two litters of living young, and died pregnant a little over one year after the operation, con- taining a third litter (Figs. to-15). Had she not been operated upon, her young by this male would undoubtedly have been albinos, for albino guinea-pigs produce only albino young, as several investigators have clearly shown. But those young which she did bear were without exception black, which character clearly they owed to the fact that they developed from eggs produced by the ovary taken at a very immature stage from a black animal. From evi- dence such as this it is concluded that the inheritance can not be affected by modifications of the body of the parent, not even when the body is completely changed, since the body, so far as heredity is concerned, is merely a container of the reproductive cells. Yo modify the inheritance we must modify the reproductive cells. But the reproductive cells are not simple; they are really dual in character, made up of equivalent parts derived from father and mother. On this matter breeding experiments throw light. If a black guinea-pig of pure race is mated with an albino, the offspring are all black, yet contain albinism as a latent or recessive character. For if one of these black offspring is now mated with the same albino, only half of the offspring are black, the others being albinos. And if two of the cross-bred blacks are mated with each other, one-fourth of the young, on the average, are albinos, three- Fic. 7.—A young, black guinea-pig, about three weeks old. Ovaries taken from an animal like this were transplanted into the albino shown immediately below it. Fic. 8.—An albino female guinea-pig. Its ovaries were removed and in their place were introduced ovaries from a black guinea-pig, like the one shown in Fig. 7. Fic. 9.—An albino male guinea-pig, with which was mated the albino shown in Fig. 8. Fics. 10-15.—Pictures of three living guinea-pigs (10-12) and of the pre- served skins of three others (13-15), all of which were produced by the pair of albinos shown in Figs. 8 and o. The Method of Evolution 45 fourths being black (Figs. 16-19). This result is explained in the following way. The cross-bred black individual received from its black parent the character black (B), and from its white parent the character white (W). In it accordingly black and white were associated together, but only the former was manifested, the white remaining hidden by the black. But in reproduction the cross-bred black individual transmits black and white in separate cells. And since these two kinds of cells are in the long run equally numerous, it follows that the cross-bred black individuals produce both black and white offspring in proportions fairly constant (Fig. 20). Inheritance of this sort is called Mendelian, after Gregor Mendel who first observed and explained it. The law governing such inheritance is called Mendel’s law. Such inheritance is satisfactorily accounted for by the assump- tion that the reproductive cells are at first dual in nature, but become simple before they can function in the pro- duction of a new individual. For this assumption we have abundant evidence furnished by the direct study of the reproductive cells with the microscope. These cells, like the cells of the body in which they are contained, show in their nuclei at cell division a fairly constant number of bodies known as chromosomes. In the worm Ascaris there are only 2 of these chromosomes; in the sea-urchin Toxo- pneustes there are 36; in mice and men, about 24. A new individual arises, in sexually produced animals, out of the union of an egg with a sperm. The sperm is relatively small, but its influence equals that of the much larger egg, which fact throws light on the nature of the material basis of heredity. It suggests, namely, that this material consists largely of ferment-like bodies which Heredity and Eugenics 46 ‘QI “SI Ul UMOYs sjeurue aya Aq paonpoid SunoA inoy yo dnoiz y—'61 ‘ory “LI put gr ‘s3Iq orvdwog =*31d-eaums ourqe uv jo : “QI ‘SI Ul WMOYs 9804} OFT] SuNcA ~— uv _ YOrTq & Jo Bunod dn-umoi3 oy} JO OMY—'SI “Ol yoryq Jo ray yey ‘sid-eoums oem ourqye Uy—' LI ‘oly ‘Sunod Joy pue ‘sid-vaums o[emay HOeIq Y—9I ‘oly The Method of Evolution 47 initiate specific metabolic processes in a suitable medium represented by the larger portion of the egg. In egg and sperm, before their union, the chromosome number is reduced one-half, from the double to the single condition; in Ascaris, from 2 to 1; in the sea-urchin, from 36 to 18; in the mouse and in the man from 24 to 12. These reductions occur in what is called the maturation of the sexual products. In the male, the primitive germ cell containing the double or 2V number of chromosomes divides up (+) () into a group of four cells, Se a te ° (*|*) ‘aveord each containing the single or : N number of chromosomes. fe See ce e This comes about by a fail- ea Jono aawcres ure of the chromosomes to ay : ea split at one of the two cell oS ae divisions which produce the ; group of four sperm cells, as they regularly do in Fic. 20.—Diagram to explain the result ° a veise shown in Figs. 16-19. ordinary cell division. A tadpole-like sperm now arises from each of the cells con- taining the reduced number of chromosomes. In the maturation of the egg, reduction is likewise accomplished by two cell divisions, in one of which the chromosomes do not split as in ordinary cell divisions. The divisions of the egg, however, are into parts of very unequal size, only one of which is fertilized, the rest failing to develop. For example, in the marine worm Nereis, according to Wilson, the maturation of the egg occurs simultaneously with its fertilization. The first maturation division separates off a minute cell, known as the first polar ZYGOTES 2 2 S S 48 Heredity and Eugenics body, and is quickly followed by a second likewise unequal division, by which a second polar body is produced. The number of chromosomes remaining in the egg nucleus is now reduced to half that in the egg before maturation. A sperm entering the egg has formed within it a second nuclear body which, like that of the egg contains a reduced number of chromosomes. By the union of these two nuclei a new nucleus is formed which contains the double number, and from this all cells of the new individual are directly derived. In all such cells the double chromosome number is present. Similar events take place in the maturation and fertilization of animal eggs in general. Now, when the egg of a black guinea-pig is fertilized with the sperm of a white one, or vice versa, protoplasmic con- stituents unite which in one case are able to produce a black coat, in the other a white one. These constituents, whatever they are, evidently separate from each other and pass into different cell products when the germ cells of the cross-bred individual ripen. It seems natural to suppose that the separation occurs at the reduction of the chromo- somes from the double to the single condition. Half the sperms, accordingly, of the cross-bred black individual bear black, half white, none both; and the same is true of the eggs (Fig. 20). Experiment proves conclusively that this is so. Blackness and whiteness behave in crosses like indi- visible units. They may be brought together repeatedly in crosses, but always separate again at the maturation of the gametes. We call them wnzt-characters. Black is a positive unit (presence of black pigment), white its corre- sponding negative (absence of black pigment). Other unit-characters are quite independent, in their inheritance, of black and white. Thus, the coat of a did-voum3 ojyar ‘Ysnor W—'ts “Oly sid-voumn3 yep ‘yJoowWs W—'1z “ol Sid-vaumns yep ‘ysnor1 y—'bz ‘oly sid-voumn3 oyYM ‘YJOous Y—'ez “OL 49 The Method of Evolution ‘uolajas Aq pasvaioul A]}eeI3 UsEq Sey BULIATIS YJ, “SoUO Yep YUM possodssaqul sey oq surejuod yeoo ayy, “std-vaumS pasaayis y—'gz “Oly S1d-voutn3 pajzjods ‘yqjoows ‘pomey-Bu0] W—"Lz “oly sid-eoumns ayy ‘Ysnoi ‘pasey-Buojy y—"9z ‘OL Sid-voums oyYA ‘yOouIs ‘pomrey-Buo] W—"Sz “Oly sairee 8 So Q 3 5 2 S ad S ny 92 The Method of Evolution 51 guinea-pig may be either rough or smooth (Figs. 21-24). Rough is a unit-character dominant over smooth in crosses, and among the second generation offspring from such a cross occur three rough individuals to one smooth one. If the rough parent is white (Fig. 23) and the smooth one dark (Fig. 21), the parents differ in two unit-characters, and the sequel shows that these are independent units. For although the immediate offspring are all dark and rough (Fig. 24), the next generation contains four sorts of indi- viduals, representing all possible combinations of the two alternative pairs of units: (1) smooth dark, like one grand- parent (Fig. 21); (2) rough white, like the other (Fig. 23); (3) rough dark, like the parents (Fig. 24); and (4) smooth white, a new combination (Fig. 22). Again, length of the hair is independent of its color or roughness (Figs. 25-27). A short-haired colored animal mated with a long-haired white one produces only short- haired colored offspring, which, bred inter se, produce in the next generation young of four sorts: (1) long white, (2) short dark, (3) long dark, and (4) short white. Recombinations in such ways can be accounted for if we suppose each different unit-character to have its basis in a different material body within the cell, perhaps in a different chromosome or part chromosome. Thus, suppose hair length to have its basis in one cell structure, which we may represent in a diagram (Fig. 29) by a circle, and sup- pose hair color to have its basis in another cell structure represented by a square, and suppose further that these two structures are independent of each other in their fusions and segregations; then if we cross a long-haired white (LW) animal with a short-haired dark one (SD), a combina- tion (or zygote) will be formed showing only the dominant 52 Heredity and Eugenics characters, short and dark hair, but able to transmit the alternative conditions, long and white hair. At repro- duction by such an individual, LZ will separate from S, and W will separate from D, passing into a different cell prod- uct, but it will be a matter of chance whether L is asso- ciated with JW as originally, or with D. Hence the chances are even for the production of the four kinds of gametes shown in the diagram, LW, SD, LD, and SW, the visible expression of which would oe produce individuals in character, respectively long — a white, short dark, long dark, apc and short white, as actually obtained by experiment. K 2 If the individuals crossed are pure and differ in three EXeCHCHCA omeve particulars, color, length, Fic. 29.—Diagram to show the result and roughness of the coat, of crossing a long-haired, white guinea- then their grandchildren pig (L W) with a short-haired, dark one w]] be of eight sorts, repre- ean senting all possible combi- nations of three independent unit-character pairs, each of which has its basis in a different material body in the cell. A great many of the characters of animals and plants behave as simple units in heredity, yielding a 3:1 ratio of dominant to recessive individuals in the second genera- tion from the cross. I have shown that this is true of certain hair characters in guinea-pigs, namely, blackness, roughness, and length of the hair. We have no idea how numerous such characters are until they happen to be lost in one individual or another. Then a new variation, a sport or mutation, is observed. It is by this means, acci- The Method of Evolution 53 dental loss of simple unit-characters, that the great color variation of domesticated animals has arisen. We should naturally consider the color character of a wild mouse, rat, or rabbit, to be very simple, for we observe such animals to breed very true to color, but the behavior of the wild type in crosses shows it in reality to be very com- plex, and to be the result of the simultaneous presence of some half-dozen or more wholly independent unit- characters. New color varieties have arisen by loss or modifi- cation of one or more of these unit-characters. For example, the wild house mouse by simple loss of three independent factors has given rise to seven additional varieties known among fancy mice. The gray fur contains black, brown, and yellow pig- ments disposed in a definite pattern in the individual hairs. Loss of this pattern alone produces the black variety. Loss of black produces the cinnamon variety; loss of both pro- duces the brown or chocolate variety. Loss of the power to produce color, that is loss of some general color factor, produces an albino, whose breeding capacity will vary with the number of other factors which it retains. Several other color factors occur in mice, the loss of which has produced new series of color varieties, but these will suffice to show the process by which new varieties arise through loss of unit-characters. Simple unit-characters are not confined to the super- ficial parts of an animal, as for example to its fur. We know these superficial characters best probably simply because they are most easily observed. Loss of horns in cattle behaves as a dominant unit- character; likewise in man a shortened condition of the skeleton producing two-jointed instead of three-jointed 54 Heredity and Eugenics fingers behaves as a simple dominant unit-character. A curious affection of the nervous system producing the waltzing condition of Japanese mice, behaves as a recessive unit-character in crosses with the normal condition. Is all inheritance unit-character inheritance? This question cannot at present be answered fully, but many facts indicate that it is. A large class of seemingly uncon- formable cases which presented the greatest obstacle to such a view has recently been brought into line with the unit- character hypothesis. I refer to cases of blending inherit- ance, in which the offspring are intermediate between the parents, and this intermediate condition persists into the next generation. Size and skeletal proportions are inherited apparently in this fashion. It is possible, however, that even in such cases unit-character segregations may really occur, though their presence is obscured because dominance does not occur. For in plants such size segregations have been observed recently by my colleague Dr. East and by others. A single illustration will suffice. When varieties of maize (or Indian corn) are crossed which differ in size of ear, the hybrid plants bear ears of intermediate size but not more variable than the more variable parent. The second generation offspring, however, are extremely variable, ranging in size from that of the smaller parent variety to that of the larger. The peculiarity of what we have called blending inheritance lies partly in the entire absence of dominance. In blending inheritance a unit-character represented once in the fertilized egg has only half as much effect as one rep- resented twice. In color inheritance, usually, but not always, a single dose of a unit-character is as effective as a double The Method of Evolution 55 dose in causing the development of the character. In size inheritance, however, the single and double doses probably produce very different effects. A further apparent difficulty encountered in interpreting blending inheritance as unit-character inheritance lies in the multiplicity of the units involved, so that segregations do not occur into a few discontinuous size classes, but into classes so numerous and differing so little from each other that it is very difficult to distinguish them. Suppose that in crosses of black with white guinea-pigs, black were represented by fwo unit-characters B and B’, instead of by one, residing perhaps in different chromosomes, and that either one of these could by itself produce black color, then a larger proportion than three-fourths of the second generation offspring would be black, namely, {+}. If, further, the presence of a larger number of factors for black produced more black pigment in the fur than a smaller number produced, then we should have gradations of black- ness among the second generation offspring as follows: Aes Soe: Add a third factor for black in the supposed cross, located perhaps in a third chromosome, and the pure whites would be reduced to 1 to 64 of the second generation off- spring, while the different gradations or intensities of black- ness would become 6, 5, 4, 3, 2, 1,0. The occasional white individual would now differ so little from the lightest black one that the two might often be confused, and there would seem to exist all intermediate stages between pure white and pure black, without entire segregation into either. By selecting for the lightest or the darkest condition within a mixed race of such second generation offspring one would obtain with each selection a larger proportion of extremely 56 Heredity and Eugenics dark or extremely light individuals, until a pure race was obtained. Further, if the black character should become attached to additional material bodies in the cell (chromo- somes or the like), so that it would be represented by addi- tional units, then the occurrence of light-colored progeny would become still rarer, and deeper intensities of black- ness than before existed would now occur. Thus selection would become a means for the modification of a character really dependent upon the inheritance of unchanging units. Now this is perhaps what occurs when one seeks to modify size by selection. There are strong reasons for believing that mendelizing characters can be modified by selection, though this idea is vigorously denied by many Mendelians, as for example by Johannsen. In Johannsen’s view, selection can do nothing but sort out variations already existing in a race. I prefer to think with Darwin that selection can do more than this, that it can heap up quantitative variations until they reach a sum total otherwise unattainable, and that it thus becomes creative. I will describe briefly certain experiences of my own which support this idea. In several cases I have observed characters at first feebly manifested gradually improve under selection until they became established racial traits. Thus in guinea-pigs, the hind-foot commonly bears three toes (Fig. 30, A). But several years ago I observed an individual which had an imperfectly developed fourth toe, similar to that shown in Fig. 30,C. From the descendants of this animal, obtained by inbreeding and selection, was formed a race having well-developed fourth toes (Fig. 30, B) on both hind feet. The extra toe made its appearance poorly devel- oped on the left foot only. About 6 per cent of the The Method of Evolution 57 offspring of this animal by normal unrelated mothers were polydactylous, but among his offspring were some with better developed fourth toes than the father possessed. Such individuals were selected throughout five successive generations, at the end of which time a good four-toed race had been established. It was found in general that those animals which had best-developed fourth toes transmitted the character most strongly in crosses with unrelated normal animals. The percentage of polydactylous individuals produced in such crosses varied all the way from o to 100 Blige Fic. 30.—A, hind-feet of an ordinary guinea-pig; B, of a four-toed guinea- pig; C, of an imperfectly four-toed guinea-pig. A per cent. By selection this percentage was increased, as was also the degree of development of the fourth toe in crosses. Another character which made its appearance among our guinea-pigs, at first feebly expressed, was a silvering of the colored fur, due to interspersing of white hairs with the colored ones (Fig. 28). The first individuals observed to have this character bore white hairs on the under surface of the body only. By inbreeding, a homozygous strain of the silvered animals was soon obtained, one in which all the offspring were silvered to a greater or less extent. Selection 8 Heredity and Eugenics mn was now directed toward two ends: (1) to secure animals which were free from spots of red or white, a condition which was present in the original stock; and (2) to secure extensive and uniform silvering on a black background. In both these objects good progress has been made. We have animals which are silvered all over the body except on a part of the head, and the percentage of such well- silvered individuals is relatively high. A more extensive selection experiment is one in which Ihave been assisted by Dr. John C. Phillips (Figs. 3rand32). Fic. 31.—Diagram showing variation in the color pattern of hooded rats. Numerals indicate arbitrary grades used. Selection in this case has been directed toward a modifica- tion of the color pattern of hooded rats, a pattern which is known to behave as a recessive Mendelian character in crosses with either the self (totally pigmented) condition or the so-called Irish (white bellied) condition found in some other rats. The extreme range of variation among our hooded rats at the outset of this experiment is indicated by the grades —2 and +3 of Fig. 31. Selection was now made of the extreme variates in either direction and these were bred separately. Two series of animals were thus The Method of Evolution 59 established: one of narrow-striped animals, minus series; the other of wide striped, plus series. In each generation the most extreme individuals were selected as parents; in the narrow series, those with narrowest stripe; in the wide series, those with widest stripe. ese ee ae | oe ieriieert eee +3 lier ST A ee ee eel | +2 E — +41 10) ={ r——— A’ f—— ag 3 = ee ~-}---[- + -2 = ws Generation 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Fic. 32.—Chart showing effects of selection in eight successive generations upon the color pattern of hooded rats. A, A’, average condition of selected parents; B, B’, of their offspring. The result of the selection is shown graphically in Fig. 32 (compare Table I). The offspring in the narrow series became with each generation narrower; those in the wide series became with each generation wider, with a single exception. In the second generation the wide stock was enlarged by the addition of a new strain of animals. This caused a temporary falling off in the average grade of the young, the two series overlapping for that generation. No 60 Heredity and Eugenics new stock was at any other time introduced in either series, the two remaining distinct at all times except in the second generation. It will be observed that a change in the aver- age grade of the parents is attended by a corresponding change in the average of the offspring, and likewise in the range of variation in the offspring. The amount of varia- bility of the offspring is not materially affected by the selection, but the average about which variation occurs is steadily changed, as are also the limits of the range of variation. TABLE | RESULTS OF SELECTION FOR MODIFICATION OF THE COLOR-PATTERN OF Hoopep Rats Generation ae) Saat) Mee Plus series 1..... eR ERGSE ESS | B56 2.05 150 Ba ibid ing Sin ae. e Se. esen 2.51 1.92 471 Spa eee ae oaa Rates 2.93 2-51 341 Ae eanduciead Sh Gna te 3.09 2.92 444 Shs Sviosekes ava esap seuaneanige eto 3-33 2.90 610 DU ESAES the Steet oer ee oes 3.00 834 Pe ee ee ee 3.83 2.14 874 a ee ee Oe ee ee 3.05 3.30 gt MO Balls Svansetsh easrsh assed ueue actors. craileiersthedete stesinus elerats le, pea ea oa Ne 3,815 Mantis Seriesiits.ic Nate La ateetletier ae 1.46 1.00 55 BS iu Aventis te a ree 1.41 1.07 132 eer ie ee re 1.56 1.18 195 AER ASCE SO OS Co 1.69 1.28 320 Gaba ee wehammele see Lave 1.41 7OI Ose ceca, eet pee AON Ser 1.86 1.56 1,252 DERE ITEE ede Ree aR aE 2.00 1.70 1,544 Oita ae orn ATG 2.03 r.78 pee Ota scescieaceusta cg apacataneevdents wed Anil latinas abcnpesiatsaeiods | eer ct 4,921 The interesting feature of this experiment is the pro- duction, as a result of selection, of wholly new grades, in the narrow series of animals having less pigment than any known type other than the albino; in the wide series, of The Method of Evolution 61 animals so extensively pigmented that they would readily pass for the “Irish type,’ which has white on the belly only, but which is known to be in crosses a Mendelian alter- native to the hooded type. By selection we have practi- cally obliterated the gap which originally separated these types, though selected animals still give regression toward the respective types from which they came. But this regression grows less with each successive selection and ulti- mately should vanish, if the story told by these statistics is to be trusted. As yet there is no indication that a limit to the effects of selection has been reached. From the evidence in hand we conclude that Darwin was right in assigning great importance to selection in evolution; that progress results not merely from sorting our particular combinations of large and striking unit- characters, but also from the selection of slight differences in the potentiality of gametes representing the same unit- character combinations. Accordingly we conclude that unit-characters are not unchangeable. They can be modified, and these modifica- tions come about in more than a single way. Occasionally a unit-character is lost altogether or profoundly modified at a single step. This is mutation. But more frequent and more important, probably, are slight, scarcely notice- able modifications of unit-characters that afford a basis for a slow alteration of the race by selection. Mutation, then, is true, but it is a half-truth; selection is the other and equally important half of the truth of evolution, as Darwin saw it and as we see it. CHAPTER IV HEREDITY AND SEX? The value of a domesticated animal often depends in considerable measure on its sex. ‘Therefore, if a means could be devised for controlling the sex of offspring, it would be of great economic value to the breeder. Endless attempts have been made to do this, and occasionally a claim of success has been made, but none of these claims has withstood the test of critical analysis or experiment. The hypotheses advanced to explain how sex may be con- trolled have been of the most varied character. In some the determination of sex has been supposed to inhere in the nature of the parents, in others it is referred to conditions of the gametes themselves. Relative age or vigor of the parents has been supposed to influence sex in various ways. The same idea has been advanced regarding the gametes themselves, it being supposed that early or late fertilization of the egg might influence its sex. Experimental evidence, however, as to these several hypotheses is wholly negative, when one elimi- nates other possible factors from the experiment. Every- thing points to the conclusion that sex rests in the last ‘In reading this chapter, the following definitions should be kept in mind. Gamete: a mature reproductive cell capable, on uniting with another gamete, of forming a new individual. Zygole: the new individual formed by the union of two gametes. Homozygole: an individual formed by the union of two gametes of like char- acter as regards heredity ITeterozygote: an individual formed by the union of two gametes of unlike character as regards heredity. 62 Heredity and Sex 03 analysis upon gametic differentiation, just as the color of a guinea-pig in a mixed race of blacks and whites depends upon whether the gametes which unite to produce it carry black or white. As the heterozygous black guinea-pig forms black-producing and white-producing gametes in equal numbers, so there is reason to think male-producing and female-producing gametes are formed in equal numbers by the parent, in many cases at least. But is it not possible that there may exist individuals which produce the two sorts of gametes in unequal numbers, and so would have a tendency to produce more offspring of one sex than of the other? Perhaps so, though we have no evidence that such a condition, if it does exist, is transmitted from one genera- tion to another. On this point I made experimental observations upon guinea-pigs, extending over a series of years. Oftentimes I found an individual that produced more offspring of one sex than of the other, but this was probably due merely to chance deviations from equality. I could get no evidence that the condition was inherited, though the experiment was continued through as many as seven generations, including several hundred offspring. The essential difference between a female and a male individual is that one produces eggs, the other sperm. All other differences are secondary and dependent largely upon the differences mentioned. If in the higher animals (birds and mammals) the sex glands (i.e., the egg-producing and sperm-producing tissues) are removed from the body, the superficial differences between the sexes largely disappear. In insects, however, the secondary sex-characters seem to be for the most part uninfluenced by presence or absence of the sex glands. Their differentiation occurs independ- ently though simultaneously with that of the sex glands. 64 Heredity and Eugenics The egg or larger gamete (the so-called macro-gamete) in all animals is non-motile and contains a relatively large amount of reserve food material for the maintenance of the developing embryo. This reserve food material it is the function of the mother to supply. In the case of some animals, for example flatworms and mollusks, the food supply of the embryo is not stored in the egg cell itself, but in other cells associated with it, and which break down and supply nourishment to the developing embryo derived from the fertilized egg. Again, as in the mammals, the embryo may derive its nourishment largely from the maternal tissues, the embryo remaining like a parasite within the maternal body during its growth, feeding by absorption. But in all cases alike the mother supplies the larger gamete and the food material necessary to carry the zygote through its embryonic stages. The father, on the other hand, furnishes the bare hereditary equipment of a gamete, with the motor apparatus necessary to bring it into contact with the egg cell, but without food for the developing embryo pro- duced by fertilization. The gamete furnished by the father is therefore the smaller gamete, the so-called micro-gamete. From the standpoint of metabolism, the female is the more advanced condition; the female performs the larger function, doing all that the male does in furnishing the material basis of heredity (a gamete), and in addition supplying food for the embryo. As regards the reproduc- tive function, the female is the equivalent of the male organism, plus an additional function, that of supplying the embryo with food. When we come to consider the structural basis of sex, we find reasons for thinking that here, too, the female individual is the equivalent of the male plus an additional element. The conclusion has very Heredity and Sex 65 naturally been drawn that if a means could be devised for increasing the nourishment of the egg or embryo, its develop- ment into a female should be thereby insured, while the reverse treatment should lead to the production of a male. But in practice this a-priori expectation is not fulfilled. Better nourishment of the mother may lead to the produc- tion of more eggs, but not of more female offspring, as ha¢ repeatedly been demonstrated by experiment. Also poor nutrition of the mother may diminish the number of eggs which she liberates, but will not increase the proportion of males among the offspring produced. An excellent summary of evidence on this point was made by Cuénot in 1900. Attempts to influence the sex of an embryo or larva by altered nutrition of the embryo or larva itself have proved equally futile. Practically the only experimental evidence of value in favor of this idea has been derived from the study of insects, and this is capable of explanation on quite different grounds from those which first suggest themselves. It has sometimes been observed, as by Mary Treat for example, that a lot of insects poorly fed produce an excess of males. In such lots, how- 7 ever, the mortality is commonly high, and more females die than males, because the female is usually larger and requires more food to complete its development. The fallacy in concluding from such evidence that scanty nutrition causes individuals which would otherwise become females to develop into males was indicated years ago by Riley. Nevertheless an argument for the artificial control of sex based on such evidence is from time to time brought forward, as, for example, a few years since by Schenk. The lates. advocate of sex control by artificial means is an Italian. Russo (1909). He claims in the case of rabbits that by 66 Heredity and Eugenics feeding the mother on lecithin or by injections of lecithin, the proportion of female births may be increased. His evidence in support of this claim is, however, wholly inade- quate, and two independent repetitions of his experiments, made by Basil in Italy and by Punnett in England, have given entirely negative results. An alternative hypothesis concerning the determination of sex has been steadily gaining ground during the last ten years, that sex has its beginning in gametic differentiation and is finally determined beyond recall in the fertilized egg by the nature of the uniting gametes. Instructive in this connection is a study of parthenogenesis, reproduction by unfertilized eggs. But before entering upon this, it may be well to review briefly the changes which regularly take place in the egg which is to be fertilized, and compare with this the changes which occur in eggs not to be fertilized. In each cell of the ordinary animal there occurs a charac- teristic number of bodies called chromosomes. We do not know that they are any more important than other cell constituents, but we know their history better. These are contained in the nucleus of the cell, and at the time of nuclear division they are found at the equator of the division spindle. There each of them regularly splits in two, and one derivative goes to either end of the spindle, and so into one of the daughter nuclei. Thus each new nucleus has, as a rule, the same chromosome composition as the nucleus from which it was derived. But the egg which is to be fertilized undergoes two nuclear divisions in succession, in only one of which do the chromosomes split. In the other division the chromo- somes separate into two groups without splitting, and each group goes into a different cell product. Consequently, Heredity and Sex 67 in each of these products the number of chromosomes is reduced to half what it is in the cells of the parental body. Thus in the egg of the mouse, by maturation, the number of chromosomes becomes reduced from about twenty-four to about twelve. Similar changes occur in the developing sperm cell (Fig. 33, upper row). Starting with the double or 2NV chromosome number, there are formed by two nuclear divisions, with only one splitting of chromosomes, four cells, each with the reduced or simplex number of chro- (@) q (x) Q® me ra mosomes, V. From each of WW a these four cells arises a tadpole-like sperm. Con- O ee a) sequently, when the sperm (nv) (w)(n) enters the egg at fertiliza- ye zs tion it brings in a group Fic. 33.—Top row, normal spermato- of NV chromosomes (in the genesis; lower row, spermatogenesis of mouse apparently 12), a a which, added to the egg contribution of NV chromosomes, brings the number in the new organism again up to 2N (in the mouse 24). Now, as regards the maturation of parthenogenetic eggs, those which are to develop without having been fertilized, three categories of cases deserve separate discussion. The simplest of these in many respects is found among the social hymenoptera (ants, bees, and wasps) (see Fig. 34, left column). The eggs are, so far as we can discover, all of a single type. They undergo maturation in the manner already described, the chromosomes being reduced to the N or simplex number. The eggs of most animals, after they have undergone reduction, are incapable of develop- 68 Heredity and Eugenics ment unless fertilized, but those of the hymenoptera may develop either fertilized or unfertilized. In the former case a female is produced, in the latter a male. The simplex or V condition is in this case the male, the duplex or 2 BEE ROTIFFR APHID (FAVORABLE f Fic. 34.—Sex determination in par- thenogenesis. Top row, nuclear condition of the parthenogenetic mother; second row, of her eggs when they develop with- out nuclear reduction, having formed a single polar cell; third row, condition of the eggs after complete maturation—the unfertilized egg in each case produces a male; fourth row, nuclear condition of the fertilized egg, always a female. condition is the female, naturally the one of higher metabolic activity, the one which forms the macro- gametes. There is a peculiarity in the maturation of the sperm cells of male animals of this sort (Fig. 33, lower row). The cells of the male are in this case already in the re- duced or simplex condition, N. In the production of the sperms the reducing division is omitted so far as nuclear components are concerned, so that each sperm formed contains the full simplex chromosome number, V. If it were less, the gamete formed would perhaps not be capable of transmitting all the hereditary characteristics of an individual. A second category of cases (Fig. 34, middle column) is represented by such simple aquatic organisms as rotifers and small crustacea, like Daphnia. In these partheno- genesis occurs exclusively when the food supply is very abundant and conditions are otherwise favorable, whereas Heredity and Sex 69 reproduction by fertilized eggs occurs only when external conditions, including food supply, are not good. Under favorable conditions only female offspring are produced. The conclusion has naturally but erroneously been drawn that good nutrition in itself favors the production of females in animals generally, which is not true. The egg produced by Daphnia, or by a rotifer, under optimum conditions does not undergo reduction (Fig. 34, second row). It remains in the 2N condition, forming but a single polar cell. It is therefore unprepared for fertilization, and in fact it is not fertilized. Its sex is like that of the animal which formed it, female. Under unfavorable conditions, however, the eggs of the rotifer and of Daphnia do not begin development until they have undergone maturation. They are also of two sizes (Fig. 34, third row)—small eggs, which develop without fertilization and which form males, and large eggs, which require fertilization, and which form females. In this category of cases, as in that of the hymenoptera, the egg which develops in the 2 condition, either from failure of reduction to occur in maturation or from fertilization following reduction, forms a female; whereas the egg which develops in the V condition forms a male. In a third category of cases there is a quantitative difference in chromatin between male and female, just as in the foregoing cases, but this does not amount to a whole set of chromosomes, V, but to only a partial set, one or two chromosomes (Fig. 34, right column). This category of cases occurs in plant lice (aphids and phylloxerans); evidence of its existence rests chiefly on recent observations made by von Baehr and Morgan. Females are formed by parthenogenesis without reduction, occurring under favorable conditions, just as in the case of rotifers. Females are also 70 Heredity and Eugenics formed by fertilization following reduction under unfavor- able conditions, just as in rotifers. In both cases the female is2.V. Males arise only by parthenogenesis under unfavor- able conditions, just as in rotifers, but the reduction which occurs before development begins is partial only. A whole set, NV, of chromosomes is not eliminated in maturation, but only 1 or 2 chromosomes. Hence the male condition here is 2V—1 or —2. The condi- tion of the gametes formed, however, is NV in both sexes. In spermatogenesis, division of the germ cells takes place into VN and N—1 daughter cells, but the latter degene- rate (like the non-nucleated cells of the bee and wasp), and only the former produce spermatozoa. Hence in ferti- lization only 2N zygotes are produced, which are invari- ably female. Summarizing the three Fic. 35.—Diagram of sex determi- categories described, we may nation when the female is homozygous, say that in all known cases of parthenogenesis, the female is in the duplex (2) condition, the male in the simplex (Ny o ) or partially duplex condition (2 —t, or 2N ==9)). ihe female in all cases has the greater sr ONCE content. In a great many insects and other arthropods, which are not parthenogenetic, it is known that, although the male, like the female, develops only from a. fertilized egg, nevertheless the male possesses fewer chromosomes than the male heterozygous. ——— Heredity and Sex vB the female. In such cases the female forms, as in cases of parthenogenesis, only N gametes, but the male forms gametes of two sorts, N and N—1 or N—2 (Fig. cio In consequence zygotes of two sorts result, those which are 2N, female, and those which are 2V—1 or 2V—2, male. Thus in the squash bug, Anasa tristis, according to Wilson, the mature egg contains rr chromosomes, the spermatozoa either to or 11 chromosomes, the two sorts being equally ee ee numerous. sa es Egg r1-++sperm 11 produces a zygote 22 (2), a female Egg 1r+sperm ro produces a zygote 21 (2N-1), a male N in this species=11; 2NV=22, the female: 2N —1=21, the male. Males and females are therefore approximately equal in number, as in most animals where the two sexes are not subject to unequal mortality. In the Mendelian sense the female is in such cases a homozygote, the male a heterozygote. The sex of an individual in such cases depends upon which sort of a sperm chances to enter the egg. But the experimental evidence indicates that both as regards sex and as regards heritable characters correlated with sex, these relations may in some cases be reversed, the female being heterozygous, the male homozygous. In such cases there is reason to think that structurally the male is 2N but the female 2V+. That is, the female is still the equivalent of the male plus some additional element and function. A structural basis in the chromosomes for such a condition has been described by Baltzer in the case of the sea-urchin. He found the regular duplex number of chromo- somes in the male; but in the female, while the number was | the same, one of the chromosomes was larger than its mate, | having an extra or odd element attached to it. In such a 42 Heredity and Eugenics case the gametes formed by the male would all be NV, but those formed by the female would be of two sorts equally numerous, viz., V and V+ (Fig. 36). Egg WN fertilized by sperm N would produce a zygote 2N, a male; egg N+ fertilized by sperm V would produce a zygote 2V+, a female. Hence, here as in other animals, the sexes would be approximately equal, but. the sex of a particular indi- vidual would depend upon which sort of egg gave rise ©). | ©), — g ~~ Upon the existence, as in US ‘ N the foregoing cases, of an a 4 | unpaired or odd_ structural a V1 element in the egg, may per- \ of a curious sort of heredity ) known as sex-limited heredity. (@)? (@)8 Everyone who knows any- thing about poultry is ac- Fic. 36.—Diagram of sex determi- quainted with the popular nation when the female is heterozy- American breed called barred gous, the male homozygous. Plymouth Rock. In this breed the feathers are marked with alternate bars of darker and lighter black. Pure barred Rocks breed true, but when crossed with other breeds, the male proves to be homozy- gous, the female heterozygous in barring. For the male Rock crossed with a non-barred breed produces only barred offspring in both sexes, but the female Rock crossed with the same non-barred breed produces offspring approxi- mately half of which are barred, the other half being non-barred. Further, the barred individuals in_this cross 4 i haps depend the explanation Heredity and Sex 73 are invariably males, the non-barred ones being females. Accordingly, the distribution of barring and non-barring in the cross is sex limited. The barred offspring produced by a cross between barred Plymauth Rocks and a non-barred breed, whether those offspring are males or females, prove to be heterozy- gous in barring, as we should expect, the- barring factor having been received only from one parent, the barred one. Further, the non-barred_offspring produced by a_barred Rock female crossed with a non-barred breed, do not trans- mit barring, hence they are pure recessives as regards , barring. Hence, also, we are forced to conclude, as alread suggested, the female of the pure barred Rock breed heterozygous as regards barring, and transmits the char. only to her male offspring, her female offsprin father is non-barred), neither being themselves b “being able to transmit barring. A pure ‘Plymouth Rock race breeds tru merely because all its males are pure, for not pure. This is shown by the folloyfng experiment. If a heterozygous barred male, produce a cross between a Rock and a non-barred breed, is#rossed with barred females, either those of a pure Rockgface or those produced by a cross, the result is the same#f¥ The male offspring are all barred; the females, half them barred, half non- barred. This result shows th -all’ barred females alike are heterozygous in barring. | Sex-limited inheritance such as this finds at the “nts| o_barring females are time its most probable explanation in the existence in the egg of an extra or plus element never found in the sperm, this element pairing with the sex-limited character in the reduction division. Thus, in the barred Rock, calling 74 Heredity and Eugenics barring B, the male of pure race is plainly BB and every sperm is B. But the female clearly contains only one B and cannot be made to contain two. Perhaps a second B is kept out by some structural element, XY, the distinctive structural element of the female individual. Then the eggs will be of two sorts: B and X (Fig. 37). Since the sperms are all B, the first type of egg when fertilized will FEMALE Mae contain BB, a homozy- gous barred individual and a male, since it lacks Y; the second type will contain BX, a bird heterozygous in B Bite barring, and a female, / since it contains X. This agrees with the experimental result. A heterozygous B\B barred male will form Fic. 37.—Diagram of sex-limited inheritance two kinds of sperm, when the female is a heterozygote, as in barred only one of which will fowls. X, female sex determiner; B, barring. can Are : contain B. If such a male be mated with a barred female, four sorts of zygotes should result as follows: Gametes of heterozygous barred male=B and — Gametes of barred female =BandX Zygotes=B - B (homozygous barred male); B - — (heterozygous barred male), B - X (barred female), and — - X (non-barred female). The observed result of this cross accords fully with the foregoing expectation. The sex-limited inheritance of barring in fowls may explained, as we have just seen, on the assumption that the Heredity and Sex 75 female is the heterozygous sex. The same is true_of sex- limited inheritance in canary birds and in the moth, Abraaas, according to Bateson and Doncaster. But these relations are exactly reversed in the pomace fly, Drosophila ampelophila according to Morgan. regards some cell structure, Y, which in the male is never Ree ee ee eee represented mor th FEMALE MaLe once. Accordingly, the formula of the RXR «rl ) female is in such cases , XX; that of the male, X—. Now the sex- | limited characters in C) Drosophila seem to be bound up with the Y structure, not repelled \ by it, as is barring in fowls. Accordingly, a XRXR XR sex-limited character ae Fic. 38.—Diagram of sex-limited inheritance may be represen ted when the female is a homozygote, as in the red- twice in the female eyed Drosophila. X, sex determiner; R, red Drosophila, but only ce once in the male; or in other words, the female may be homozygous as regards a sex-limited character, but the male can only be heterozygous (Fig. 38). Drosophila normally has red eyes, but the redness of the eye is a distinct unit-character, sex limited in heredity. Further, males are regularly heterozygous in this character, while females are homozygous. For Morgan has obtained a race in which the eyes are white, owing to the loss of the red character; and reciprocal crosses of this race with 76 Heredity and Eugenics ordinary red-eyed animals yield different results. ‘The red-eyed female crossed with a white-eyed male produces @ white-eyed female produces offspring only half of which are red eyed, viz., the females, whereas the males are white eyed. — These different results in the two cases apparently come about as follows: FIRST CASE Gametes of red-eyed female =X-R and X-R Gametes of white-eyed male =X and — Zygotes=X - X-R (red-eyed female), and —-X-R (red-eyed male). SECOND CASE Gametes of white-eyed female =X and X Gametes of red-eyed male =X-R and — Zygotes=X - X-R (red-eyed female), and — - X (white-eyed male). A short condition of the wings in Drosophila, which renders the animal incapable of flight, is likewise sex limited in heredity, as has been shown by Morgan. By crossing two races of Drosophila, each of which possessed a different sex-limited character, Morgan has been able to combine the two characters in a single race. Thus was obtained a race both white eyed and short winged. The synthesis cannot be made originally in a male individual, but only in a female. For only in the female can the two characters be brought together, cach associated with a different Y, since in the male only one Y is present. Al- though each sex-limited character seems to be attached to or bound up with an X structure, it evidently has a material basis distinct from_y. Otherwise it would_not Heredity and Sex 77 be possible for the character to leave one Y and attach itself to the other, as apparently takes place in the female when the combination of two sex-limited characters in the same gamete is secured through a cross. The combination is apparently secured in this way: Gametes uniting, X¥-R and Y—L Zygote formed, X-R - X-L Its gametes, X-R and X-L, or X-R-L and X. One of the uniting gametes, X-R, is formed by the red-eyed, short-winged parent; the other, X-L, is formed by the long-winged, white-eyed parent. The zygote result- ing is a red-eyed individual, since it contains R; it is long winged, since it contains LZ; it is a female, since it contains two Xs. Now, its gametes are of four sorts, as indicated. The first two sorts result from simple separation of the two Xs, each with its associated character, R in one case, L in the other. But the third sort could result only from the attachment of R and LZ to the same X, leaving the other X without either R or L as the fourth kind of gamete. This kind, which transmits neither red eyes nor long wings, would represent the new gametic combination—white eyed and with short wings. —The experimental evidence that gametes of these four sorts are formed by females of the origin described is as follows: When such a female is mated with a long-winged, white-eyed male, there are obtained female offspring, all of which are long winged, but half of them are red eyed, half white eyed. The male offspring, however, are of four sorts, viz., red short, white long, red long, and white short. This result harmonizes with the hypothesis advanced. For if the gametes of the female are X-R, X-L, X-R-L, 78 Heredity and Eugenics and Y, and those of the male are X-L and —, then the following combinations should result: X-L-. X-R, red long female X-L-X-L, white long female X-L - X-R-L, red long female X-L.X, white long female — - X-R, red short male ——.-X-L, white long male — -NX-R-L, red long male —.V\, white short male This expected result accords with that actually obtained by Morgan. Color-blindness in man is a sex-limited character, the inheritance of which resembles that of white eyes or short wings in Drosophila, rather than of barring in poultry. Color-blindness is much commoner in men than in women. A color-blind man, however, does not transmit color-blindness to his sons, but only to his daughters, the daughters, however, are themselves normal provided the mother was; yet they transmit color-blindness to half their sons. 308 * nab. Perhaps the easiest method of showing the combinations of gametes in Mendelian inheritance is by the use of four squares multiplied once by four for each character pair. In the case of dihybrids sixteen squares are necessary. Write each possible gametic combination of the male cells in each horizontal row of squares, AB, Ab, aB, and ab. Next write the same combinations for the female gametes in each vertical column of squares. This gives all the zygotic combinations possible. We are now ready to see just why one cannot tell whether he is dealing with a simple or complex state of affairs in Mendelian inheritance. In the monohybrid ratio of purple and non-purple aleurone cells of maize the segregation in the F, generation is: P p P P p p Three purples to one non-purple are produced. In the dihybrid ratio when other white varieties were used as one of the parents, nine purples to seven non-purples were produced. This simply means that two factors are necessary to produce the purple color. These factors may 96 Heredity and Eugenics be represented by the lettersC and P. The gametic formula of the purple variety is CP; the gametic formula of these particular white varieties is cp. If either C or P is absent from the zygotic formula of F, then the zygote is white as is shown in the diagram. DIAGRAM ILLUSTRATING INHERITANCE OF PURPLE ALEURONE CELLS IN MAIZE WHEN TWO FACTORS ARE CONCERNED CP cp | cP CP CP Cp cP cp Purple Purple Purple Purple Cp Cp Cp Cp CP Cp GP cp Purple White Purple White cP cP cP cP CP Cp cP cp Purple Purple White White cp cp cp cp GP. Cp GR. cp Purple White White White The formula of the purple is therefore CCPP, but whites with formulas CCpp, ccPP, and ccpp may exist. When the whites have formulae CCpp or ccPP they give monohybrid ratios when crossed with the purple, but when they have the formula ccpp a dihybrid results. This illustration shows both why one cannot tell just how complex a character is, and why characters essential to the species cannot be analyzed. These cases of Mendelian inheritance are types. Other Inheritance in the Higher Plants 97 cases may be more complex in appearance but this is a sur- face complexity only. They yield to similar analyses when they are properly understood. This complexity in appear- ance has many times made cases appear to be exceptions to Mendel’s general law. Various ratios have been obtained that were seemingly inexplicable, but these one by one have been found to be merely variations of the simple ratios that we have just discussed. Perhaps the most interesting of these aberrant ratios are those cases of heredity dealing with latent characters. We will consider some of these briefly as examples of the various manifestations which are found in the hereditary transmission of plant characters. One of the most important classes of latency we have just discussed. It is called latency of separation. Where a character exists through the interaction of two factors, these factors may at some time become separated, that is, possessed by different individuals. When this occurs the character is apparently lost and does not again appear unless two individuals bearing the complementary factors are crossed. This is the explanation of the old phenome- non of reversion after crossing. It was first explained by Bateson from experiments with sweet peas. He found that two white varieties yielded the purple color of the wild sweet pea when crossed. In the same way I have found that the white seeds in the dihybrid ratio of 9 : 7, shown by the purple X non-purple cross of maize varieties, give purple seeds when crossed at random. The non-purples exist in the following ratios: 1 CChp \; Copp TOT OOP le CCL Dp I ¢c cpp 98 Heredity and Eugenics When crossed at random, there are 7X6=42 possible com- binations of which 24 give all white seeds and 18 give some purple seeds. Of these 18 ears there would be on the aver- age 2 pure purples, 8 with purples and whites in a ratio of tr: 1,and 8 with purples and whites ina ratio of 1: 3. Such results have been obtained experimentally and examples are shown in Fig. 42. Tic. 42.—Reversion due to latency of separation. Purple seeds produced by crossing whites. Ear 1, 1 to 3 ratio; ear 2,1 to £ ratio. A different kind of latency is simply invisibility of a character due to some obscuring factor which may be removed by means of a cross in which the latter is absent. This latency is called that of hypostasis, a hypostatic char- acter being one which is obscured by another called the epistatic character. There are instances where this epi- static character may be removed mechanically and the hypostatic character revealed. Such a one is the red-eared maize, for the color there lies in the hull or pericarp. This Inheritance in the Higher Plants 99 may be scraped off to show the color of the true seed which may be either yellow or white. In most cases, however, the relations are physiological and the hypostatic character can be demonstrated only by crossing. For example, the maize with purple aleurone cells carries also a factor for red aleurone cells which can be demonstrated only by crossing it with a variety in which the purple factor is absent. There is produced an F, generation with a ratio of 27 purples : 9 reds : 28 whites. This statement may appear to be a direct contradiction of the interpretation of the inheritance of purple aleurone color that has just been given. In reality it is merely another illustration of the fact that one can never know definitely the factors involved in producing a character, for he can never feel assured that the parents involved in the cross have differed in all the factors that affect its develop- ment. When the purple was crossed with the white and a ratio of 9 purples : 7 whites obtained in the F, generation, it was proper to interpret the purple parent as PPCC and the non-purple parent as p pcc. But when the purple is crossed with another non-purple and a ratio of 27 purples: g reds : 28 whites is obtained in the F, generation, it is clear that a different interpretation is necessary. The purple parent has the formula P P R RCC and the white parent the formula p prrcc. The zygotic formula of a pure red seed is RRC C and of a pure purple seed is PP RRCC, but a seed with the formula P PCC is white. In other words, the factor P produces the visible purple color only in the presence of factors C and R. The purple has always the constitution P P R RCC ; when it is crossed with whites having the characters PP RRcc or PPrrCC, a ratio of 3 purples : 1 white is obtained in F,; when it is crossed 100 Heredity and Eugenics with a white having the composition P Prrcc, a ratio of 9 purples : 7 whites is obtained in F,; when it is crossed with a white having the composition p prrcc, a ratio of 27 purples : 9 reds : 28 whites is obtained in F,,. It is possible that another factorial difference may yet be found which will show this character to be still more complex. Baur, Bateson, Saunders, and Gregory have shown that the sap colors of the flowers of Antirrhinum, of Lathyrus, of Matthiola, and of Primula belong to this type and are yet more complex. But this does not affect our general conception of the inheritance of the colors in the least. Sometimes latent characters very similar to this are due to the presence of a second inherited factor which does not allow the first character to develop. This is called latency due to inhibition. Similarly there may be inherited char- acters which help to a more perfect development other independently transmitted characters. This is undoubtedly an important phase of Mendelianism for although we may conceive factors as holding within themselves the poten- tialities of certain characters, they undoubtedly are influ- enced in their development by the development of other inherited characters. One may imagine that factors repre- senting characters may be transmitted but are either not expressed at all or are developed to a limited degree owing to the presence or the absence of other inherited characters that affect this development. Perhaps this theoretical conception may be made plain by an example of what has been termed latency of fluctua- tion. In this class are included characters which are poten- tially present in an organism but which may develop to a greater or less extent due to varying influences which sur- Inheritance in the Higher Plants 101 round them. There is a red color in the pericarp or hull of the maize seed which is transmitted as a simple Mendelian monohybrid when crossed with a variety in which the character is absent, but the color develops to its full extent only in bright sunlight. When a black bag is placed over the young ear the color does not develop at all, yet such a colorless ear will transmit the color as well as if that ear had developed in bright sunlight. One may easily see then how he might be deceived in the classification of such indi- viduals and thereby draw wrong conclusions regarding the inheritance of the character. Though the example just given is one of latency of fluc- tuation, it really consists in the character being partially inhibited by absence of light. One may see from it, how- ever, that there can be real latency of inhibition through the influence of inherited factors that affect the development of independent characters. We have hitherto considered characters that seem to be transmitted independently of one another. Let us now spend a moment in discussing characters that appear to be either coupled or antagonistic to each other in their trans- mission. Some very interesting results on this subject have been obtained by Emerson (Fig. 43). There are maize varieties which are red in both the pericarp of the seed and the cob. When one of these is crossed with a variety in which color is absent in both the pericarp and cob, the second hybrid generation produces three ears like the colored parent to one ear like the white parent. One might suppose that the color in pericarp and in cob was due toa single gametic factor. This can hardly be the case, how- ever, for there are varieties with red pericarp and white cob and varieties with white pericarp and red cob. When two 102 Heredity and Eugenics such varieties are crossed together the first hybrid genera- tion produces ears with red pericarp and red cob like the colored variety in the previous cross. In the second hybrid generation, however, we find produced 1 red Heine n™a8 SHA25D ALS 240 nS) Oo nee Ae. 2v @ [iad | : DaVOQy.nV Dpem On eee te 2% MS Fic. 43.—Red pericarp and red cob coupled in inheritance. (Photo by Emerson.) F, generation shown. Ears 1, 2, 3 with red cob and red pericarp, eat 4 with white cob and white pericarp. pericarp with white cob: 2 red pericarp with red cob: 1 white pericarp with red cob. The first and third classes breed true to their conditions, but the second class proves to be always heterozygous and breaks up in the next gen- eration as did those with similar characters of the first hybrid generation. Inheritance in the Higher Plants 103 Without going into the theory of this phenomenon it is apparent that in the first case red pericarp and red cob cé e Bissccse sh Coe puGQgGs @e, , 3 c= ) ~ -— "s ak re 4 UGG a 0ae ) ge > wu é ae és pee A Tic. 44.—Red cob and red pericarp antagonistic in inheritance. (Photo by Emerson.) [2 generation shown. Ears 1 and 4, like parents, are pure types. Ears 2 and 3 are heterozygous. were not produced by a single factor in the usual sense, but by two individual factors coupled in their inheritance. In the second cross the two factors were not coupled; in fact 104 Heredity and Eugenics they were just the opposite, they were antagonistic to each other so that no pure types were produced having red peri- carps and red cobs (Fig. 44). This is an example of a fea- ture which is probably very widespread in the plant world, but of which we at present know little. It is cited for two reasons; first, to show that characters may at one time be antagonistic to each other and at another time coupled together, and second, to show that one is not able to say beforehand whether a manifestation of a character in several organs is due to one or to several separately inherited factors. Leaving out of consideration sex-limited inheritance of which little is known in plants, we have now briefly gone over simple type cases of some of the most important present- day Mendelian knowledge; but we have considered only crosses in which the potential character or characters are present in one parent and absent in the other. At least they behave that way and may reasonably be so interpreted. Such differences between parents are qualitative, but most differences between parents are quantitative and give an apparent blend in the first hybrid generation. Nearly all cases where varieties differ in the size of their organs are of this kind. Can such phenomena be interpreted by Mendelian notation? I believe they can. One may think of a factor for a character being present in the germ cell not only once but twice or even a greater number of times. If these factors are transmitted independently and are not paired with each other, but each with its own absence, one may very easily interpret size inheritance. For example, when a certain dent variety of maize is crossed with a flint variety as shown in Fig. 45, an intermediate condition is obtained in the first hybrid generation. In the second Inheritance in the Higher Plants 105 hybrid generation one ear like each original parent is obtained out of every sixteen instead of every four (Figs. 46, 47). This inheritance is therefore dihybrid in charac- ter. In lke manner, a higher number of transmissible fac- - tors may affect the development of what is to the eye a single character. Since dominance is not an essential feature of Mendeli- anism, size characters may show intermediates or blends in the first hybrid generation and still fulfil the essential conditions of Mendel’s law by recombining in such a fashion as to produce individuals like either parent in the second hybrid generation provided a sufficiently large number of individuals to allow for the recombination of several factors is grown in that generation. Such recombinations do occur and can be shown by experiment. For example, the small variety of corn, Tom Thumb, when crossed with a larger variety like the Black Mexican (Fig. 48), gives a first hybrid generation that is intermediate between the two parents. One may call this a blended condition; yet if there were blended inheritance this condition would be transmitted, while if Mendelian recombination occurred, sizes compa- rable to either parent would be obtained in the second hybrid generation. Such extremes were obtained as is shown in the figure. If the possible Mendelian interpretation of quantitative characters has been made clear, the statement that Men- del’s law is probably universally applicable where sexual reproduction occurs will not seem rash. There are still some apparent exceptions to the law, but they are so few that one may well believe we simply do not know how to bring them into line and not that they are actual exceptions. Of course it is quite likely that there are other laws which 106 Heredity and Eugenics modify the action of Mendel’s law in a manner similar to that in which the physical laws holding good under theo- retical conditions are modified under conditions existing in nature. Granted, then, that Mendelianism is broad in scope, could it have been of great value during the progress of ty eo yo e or pl win ee. Cd Se i & Ud eB Fic. 45.—Inheritance of physical condition of endosperm in maize. Parents and F generation shown. evolution? That it should have been of great value to very primitive forms of life does not seem possible, and even after the origin of sex its precise importance is somewhat problematical. It has disposed of one of the main criticisms against Darwin, at least, that of the swamping effects of intercrossing upon newly arisen characters that are an advan- tage to the specics. Since characters are segregated as units 107 Inheritance in the Higher Plants Flint- SRIBHEL> iyi in maize. ETT a tntitee, A, RAPS 52 NSDI PEDROS) DEER ORESS ATRL: DOS RAS GORTESEDODCEERSTIO TN EUS ao west, &o8 iH Oo ay 9 0G8 } =: sash {atts tra ah 28) Sys: os . id iad phipid esse wetintie * Ukiah ESO Lgs Oiod | Fy eee cc qeenstetee Fic. 46.—Inheritance of physical condition of endosperm like F; segregate above and I’; progeny below. (aa | Dent- Fic. 47.—Inheritance of physical condition of endosperm in maize. like F. segregate above and F; progeny below. 108 Heredity and Eugenics in the hybrid it matters not whether they are dominant or recessive or whether there is no dominance, there will be no dilution from intercrossing. A second and more important advantage, due to the operation of the law, results from the recombination of characters. Characters may be transmitted as units and chance recombinations of these characters may occur without anything really new to the organism being formed, yet in this recombination the organism as a whole may be better fitted for its environ- ment than ever before. And this is giving recombination its smallest value, for, however independently potential characters may be transmitted, no one believes that a developing organism is simply a mass of independently developing unit-characters. The characters of an organ- ism are more or less dependent upon one another in their expression, and in this interdependent development the greatest possibility for good recombinations occurs. Theo- retically nothing new may result from the mere act of cross- ing, but practically a new combination of old characters may result in something quite different. A little thought and the use of chemical analogies where the same mixtures produce different chemical results under different physical conditions, show the importance of this conception to Men- delian theory. Yet it is not necessary to believe the cur- rent teaching that one has only new recombinations and not new characters to deal with in crosses. The theory that several germ-cell factors may be due to produce the same character gives us a reasonable and orthodox explana- tion of the origin of characters really and truly new by the interaction of gametic factors that are old. For example, let us suppose that in a certain species with a petioled leaf there is a variety which has the presence of factor A pro- Inheritance in the Higher Plants 109 (00x) F Fic. 48.—Inheritance of length of ear in maize. Parents above, F; genera- tion in center, F, generation below. Class sizes in centimeters with number of variates below. IIO Heredity and Eugenics ducing a slight tendency toward a sessile leaf. Factors B and C may ultimately arise as modifications of factor A. Each alone may produce the same result as A; yet if they are transmitted independently, are not allelomorphic to each other and are cumulative in their action, a sessile leaf— a new character—is produced. The remainder of this chapter will be devoted to a con- sideration of Johannsen’s genotype conception of heredity. It may seem as if Mendelism has been dropped abruptly to take up anew subject. Thisisnot the case. The genotype conception of heredity is merely an acknowledgment of the universality of Mendelism. Johannsen, who developed the genotype idea, found that some variations were not inherited. These were the general variations in relative perfection of development of parts, caused by varying physical and chemical conditions of environment. This is only an acceptance of Weismann’s theory that inheritance is from germ cell to germ cell, and that ordinary environmental influences affecting the body only are not transmissible. Of course both Weismann and Johannsen acknowledge that certain changes in environ- ment may produce structural modification of the germ plasm and therefore a heritable variation, but whether the heritable variation produced is ever identical with the adaptive response of the parent organism is still in dispute. If this contention be true, it follows that the hereditary characters of an organism are determined by the consti- tution of the fertilized egg from which it came. Johannsen denotes the sum total of the gametic factors making up a zygote by the word genotype. If two individuals possess identical gametic factors, they are members of the same genotype. Of course no one can describe a genotype in Inheritance in the Higher Plants III concrete terms. It is a theoretical, a philosophical, concep- tion. If one crosses two individuals, the genes or gametic factors common to each breed true. He can obtain an idea of the behavior in heredity of those factors only which are not common to the two parents. These factors segre- gate and recombine in definite proportions. They follow Mendel’s law. The genotype conception of heredity is therefore the conception that duplex or homozygous gametic factors are due to produce identical results within the limits of variability imposed by external conditions and by the influence of other independent gametic factors during ontogeny, no matter what is the appearance of the indi- vidual from which they were derived. This is a strict Mendelian conception of heredity extended to the organism as a whole. We need not go into the many lines of work that support the genotype theory. Considered in a broad way I believe no reputable modern work is irreconcilable to it, although some authors do not so interpret their work. All modern plant breeding is in its support, for the principle of Vilmorin, the progeny test, which is the basis of all modern selection work, is founded upon the same conception. In naturally inbred plants, one has commercial strains which are mechani- cal mixtures of near-homozygotes, and can be immediately isolated. In naturally cross-bred plants or in bisexual ani- mals, one has physiological mixtures, that is, hybrids or heterozygotes, from which it takes somewhat longer to iso- late particular strains that are genotypically homozygous in respect to certain characters, but in which the separation is accomplished by the same means—the breeding test. In the theoretical homozygous genotype transmissible variations may occur, for no one believes protoplasm Tre Heredity and Eugenics unchanging. Such a conception would be mechanical and not biological. These changes must affect the germ cell structurally to be inherited. They may be large, they may be small. We may call them mutations with DeVries, or we may use the simpler and broader term, inherited varia- tions. When they occur, new varieties may be isolated in which they are present, and these may be considered to be relatively permanent as compared with the non- inherited fluctuations that continually occur due to varying environment. One may question the stability of unit-characters as does Castle, but I cannot see how this affects the truth of the genotype conception as a help toward an idea of the process of heredity. Stability isa relative thing. Why is there not a scale of stability in biology even as in chemistry? Many unit-characters are high in the scale of stability, others may be low. Certain characters ordinarily transmitted perfectly may possibly be modifiable by selection. We might imagine their factors to be huge chemical molecules, stable as a whole but modifiable by isomerism or even the dropping off or adding on of unimportant radicles. This is a smaller issue, unimportant when compared with the genotype conception as a whole. The important point as the foundation of the modern view of heredity I give in Johannsen’s own words: “Personal qualities are the reactions of the gametes joining to form a zygote; but the nature of the gametes is not deter- mined by the personal qualities of the parents or ancestors in question.” CHAPTER VI THE APPLICATION OF BIOLOGICAL PRINCIPLES TO PLANT BREEDING In this chapter I shall take up the commercial appli- cation of some of the principles of plant genetics previously discussed. The fact must again be emphasized that there are two kinds of variation: t. Fluctuating variations, which are due solely to sur- rounding influences such as better position for develop- ment or varying fertility of the soil. Such variations, are not inherited. 2. Inherited variations, which are due to some structural change in the reproductive cells. These variations may depend upon environmental conditions for their full devel- opment but not for their transmission. Inherited variations possess the only value to the plant breeder, yet the work of improving plants is rendered a great deal more irksome by the presence of fluctuations and by the fact that one cannot tell the gametic constitution of a plant, that is, its breeding capacity, by its appearance. The whole problem of the plant breeder is to find, to fix, and to recombine desirable inherited variations, and to do this in spite of their tendency to be obscured by fluctuations. The methods used to accomplish these results will be taken up presently. First, attention must be called to a physio- logical phenomenon that may be made a tool of such high value to the plant breeder that the statement just made concerning his problem is apparently untrue. This matter 113 TI4 Heredity and Eugenics is an exception, however—a thing apart from the usual breeding procedure. It will be remembered that when a plant receives iden- tical character factors from each parent, that character is homozygous and breeds true; but when the plant receives the character from only one parent, the character is hetero- zygous and shows segregation in the next generation. This heterozygous condition, though not fixable itself, since it always breaks up in the succeeding generation, is a valuable asset to the plant breeder if properly utilized and a distinct disadvantage if unrecognized. The fusion of two gametes into a zygote which is known as fertilization effects two very different results: first, a union of the hereditary factors possessed by these gametes; second, a stimulation to the cell division necessary for normal development. Probably in every case where fer- tilization can take place at all there is a certain amount of this stimulus to development, but the fact of especial interest to plant breeders is that this stimulus is generally far greater in a hybrid or heterozygote than it is in a pure-bred or homozygous individual. The stimulus is simply toward greater and quicker cell division and affects only size and rapidity of maturity. The tobacco genus (Vicotiana) furnishes an admirable type illustration of the stimulus due to heterozygosis because the species are generally self-fertilized under natural con- ditions. The stamens and the pistil are about the same length, and since the pollen is usually shed before the flower opens, self-pollination must occur unless foreign pollen is carried to the unopened bud by insects. When varieties of the common tobacco NV. tabacum are crossed together, the first hybrid generation is nearly always from 5 to 50 Application of Biological Principles to Plant Breeding 115 per cent taller than the average of the parents. The hybrids also have somewhat larger leaves than the average of the two parents although the number is generally inter- mediate. This statement is true in general for crosses between the varieties of most other species. When true species are crossed, however, the behavior of the first hybrid generation is somewhat different. Sometimes there is a great increase in vigor. WN. tabacum X N. sylvestris gives hybrids that are nearly double the average height of the parents. NV. tabacum X N. alata, on the other hand, gives hybrids that are less than one-fourth the size of the smaller parent. Both of these crosses are sterile, so that the differ- ence in behavior cannot be correlated with sterility. One can simply say that species vary in their affinity to cross with other species. All gradations are found, from those that produce full quotas of viable seed, to those where only an occasional seed is found or where the capsule simply develops without the formation of seed. And in general the additional vigor of the first hybrid generation increases with the ease of making the cross. With perfect ease of crossing the stimulus is roughly a function either of the number or of the kind of character pairs for which the individual is heterozygous. Species naturally self-fertilized lke tobacco or wheat must get along without the increased vigor due to hetero- zygosis. One notices the difference only when artificial crosses are made. Species which in nature are cross- fertilized, however, are usually heterozygous for so many characters that one does not think of their vigor as being largely due to this cause. The fact is only brought to notice when the species is self-fertilized artificially, for this tends to isolate homozygous strains (Fig. 49). 110 Heredity and Eugenics Ii, for example, a commercial variety of maize is self- (2xD F SUZS bu peracre S Nod, ae bu per acre Nod, ATT bu peroere | Fic. 49.—Genotypical strains of maize withdrawn from a commercial variety by inbreeding and their behavior when crossed fertilized for a number of generations, the plants tend to become homo- zygous, to lose the vigor due to heterozygosity and to become smaller and less productive. This loss of vigor was for years interpreted as the direct effect of self- fertilization. Now we know that it is simply the withdrawing of pure strains from hybrid com- binations. In a few generations the strains become practically pure and the loss of vigor ceases. Some strains of maize still yield remark- ably well after many generations of self- fertilization. Other strains are so poor that they can scarcely be kept alive. In fact it is evident that they are kept alive merely by the increased vigor of growth due to continual natural hybridization with other strains. A pplication of Biological Principles to Plant Breeding 117 Since all commercial methods of selection in maize, as well as other naturally cross-bred species, have as their ultimate goal the isolation of good homozygous strains (for this is what the words “selection to type” mean), it is quite evident that the longer selection has been carried on the more of this stimulus due to heterozygosity or hybridity is lost. No method of breeding naturally cross-bred species therefore, where size and total yield are the main objects, is proper unless these facts are taken into consideration and the methods so modified as to utilize them. This is done by growing only the first hybrid generation of crosses between good strains. Nor is it alone in wind-pollinated field crops, such as maize, that these methods are useful. Horticultural crops such as tomatoes and eggplants can be grown from hand-hybridized seed with a profit greatly exceeding the extra cost of its production. It may be that even certain trees can be hybridized to advantage for undoubtedly Burbank’s quick-growing walnuts are due to this phenomenon. Furthermore, it accounts for the fact that all asexually propagated crops worthy commercial supremacy, such as grapes and potatoes where yield is the object of prime importance, are always hybrids. Their mode of commercial propagation is such that the first hybrid generation can be indefinitely prolonged. It is not easy to leave this subject without mentioning the important réle which this growth stimulus due to hybridity may have played in the evolution of the higher plants. In self-fertilized species, for example the violets, the fact that the hybrid between two nearly related strains was more vigorous than either parent type would have given it such an advantage in the ordinary struggles for existence against inhospitable environment, that the chances are greatly in favor of its surviving to produce recombi- 116 Heredity and Eugenics nations of the parental characters in the next generation. And there is always the chance that new recombinations of parental characters may prove better fitted to survive than the old combinations. In cross-bred species the stimulus of hybridity holds a still greater advantage since even homozygous strains that are weak and could never exist alone, may, through combination with other strains, be kept in existence as heterozygotes. For example, one finds in maize literally thousands of genotypic strains in a single commercial variety. Many could not exist alone, yet they continue to exist in commercial varieties through hybridity and their existence may be proven by their being partially withdrawn by inbreeding. Such strains may have great possibilities in certain combinations as is shown in Fig. 49. In inbred or self-fertilized species such as tobacco, however, strains weak in themselves perish and are lost to sight because there is no probability of their being hybridized and given a chance of showing their power in combination. This one phenomenon, alone, may account for the commonness of cross-fertilized species and the rarity of self- fertilized species, since it can be shown that there is no evil effect due to inbreeding per se. Passing now to the work more generally included in plant breeding, we find that commercial methods fall naturally into two classes, hybridization and selection. They are not really thus separable since one must use selection after hybridization, but in the first category are classed all cases where man produces hybrids artificially. The main object of hybridization is the shuffling of unit-characters in the first hybrid generation and their recombination and fixation in succeeding generations. The object of selection is to withdraw from mechanical mixtures or from physio- A pplication of Biological Principles to Plant Breeding 119 logical mixtures due to hybridity, strains characterized by desirable new variations. Practical procedure in hybridization naturally varies somewhat depending upon the exact object in view. I will endeavor to illustrate the following phenomena as those of most importance: (a) Recombinations of desirable char- acters and their fixations, including the production of blends; (b) production of desirable combinations in the first Fic. 50.—Buds of Nicotiana tabacum showing time and method of castration hybrid generation and their continuation by asexual propa- gation; (c) production of fixed first generation hybrids. If one is to begin at the real beginning in this discussion, he should spend a moment in describing the mechanical operations of crossing (Fig. 50). There are three important steps. First it must be determined by experiment what environmental conditions are best suited for normal seed production. Second, an intimate knowledge of the flower- ing habits and flower structure should be gained in order 120 Heredity and Eugenics that the flowers may be castrated at the correct time without injury and properly protected from foreign pollination until the time for hybridizing. Third, care must be exercised in applying the pollen of the proposed male parent, for both premature and delayed pollination inhibits seed formation. The precise conditions under which a cross should be made to be the most successful are not easily determined. The proper preparation of the breeding plot even before the plants are grown is necessary. One takes it for granted that on most soils some fertilizer will be used, for the plants must be normal to seed well. The three essential elements of soil fertility are nitrogen, potassium, and phosphorus, and to get the best results compounds of these elements must be present in proper proportions. First, available potassium must be present in quantities sufficient for the normal production of healthy roots, leaves, and stems, and a moderate excess will not be harmful. If nitrates are present in excess, however, vegetative growth will be over- stimulated and seed production will be small. A lack of phosphorus will produce the same effect upon seed pro- duction, but for a different reason. Phosphorus is an essen- tial constituent of the proteid compounds found in large quantities in the seed. If the plants are to be in the best condition for the production of good seed after crossing, therefore, the soil should contain just the right amount of nitrates for a normal vegetative growth, and a generous supply of potash and phosphates. The exact amounts must be determined by experiment for each soil and each species of plant. External conditions that are also under partial control of the breeder are available moisture through irrigation and sunlight by proper spacing or artificial shading. A pplication of Biological Principles to Plant Breeding 121 Other necessary knowledge that can be obtained only from experience is, which are the best flowers on the plant to serve as parents of the cross and what is the proper time for their pollination. For example, in the grasses the first flowers that appear usually form larger, healthier seed than the later blossoms. In most of the Solanaceae, the petunias, browallias, etc., the exact opposite is true. The time when the individual flower is most receptive to pollen is even more narrowly limited. Both premature and delayed pollina- tion is the cause of many failures and the optimum time should be accurately determined. Having exercised these precautions, it remains to study carefully the structure of the flower in order that it may be emasculated, i.e., the anthers removed before the pollen is shed, with sufficient adroitness that neither the anthers shall be opened nor the parts of the pistil injured. Only a few buds upon a single flower spike should be operated upon if they are to be given the best chance of development. If the buds are very small and some pollen unavoidably reaches them, it may be washed off with comparative safety with a dental syringe if done immediately. It is often recommended that the calyx and corolla be cut away when emasculating. This should be avoided if possible and the floral envelopes left as a protection to the pistil. After emasculation the buds should be protected from foreign pollen until time for pollination, and again after pollination at least until the fruits have begun to form (Fig. 51). This protection may be an ordinary paper bag when the crossing is done in the field. It may be used with a plug of cotton around the mouth if special precautions are found necessary. In the greenhouse a square of thin celluloid rolled around the flower and caught with two rubber bands, each end being 122 Heredity and Eugenics protected with absorbent cotton plugs, is a better device. It gives excellent protection and allows transpiration, But I must pass from the technique of hybridizing to Vic. 51.—Impaliens sullani showing protection of castrated buds in green- house. the results. They are much more interesting. As has already been stated, desirable horticultural novelties are obtained most frequently by crossing two plants which A pplication of Biological Principles to Plant Breeding 123 differ in several transmissible characters. The first hybrid generation may be as uniform as one of the pure varieties, but in the second hybrid generations all possible combina- tions of the characters of the two parents are obtained provided a sufficient number of individuals have been grown. Among these combinations many new and desir- able types may be found. Some of them are pure types; some are heterozygous and will again segregate. Since homozygous and heterozygous types are found which are exactly alike in appearance, the only way to determine which plants are pure is to self-fertilize desirable individuals and raise a third generation. For example, 1. alata, a species with large white flowers, when crossed with N. forgetiana, a species with small red flowers, gives hybrids that are very uniform in all their characters. The flowers are intermediate between those of the parents in size, and are red in color. In the second hybrid generation, there are 16 visibly different color types. Among these there are really 81 classes, including those both pure and hybrid. It is therefore necessary to grow seed from many self-fertilized plants for another generation to be certain of getting pure strains of each type. But having done this, the pure types continue to breed true in spite of the mixed ancestry. This method is typical of the manner in which floral novelties are produced. So many varieties carry latent characters that one is always likely to obtain new things in crosses. Results of greater economic worth, however, are probably obtained by combinations made for a definite purpose. A beautiful example of such work is afforded by the experiments of Biffen. English wheats have long been known as highly productive varieties, but they are very 14 Heredity and Eugenics susceptible to a fungus disease called rust, and do not make first-class bread on account of the low percentage of gluten. After many importations, wheats resistant to rust and high in gluten content were obtained, but these were not profitable because of their low yields. Biffen then went to work to analyze the transmissible characters of the wheats into Men- delian factors by a large series of crosses. This he was able to do. The rest was easy. He has now produced by hybridiza- tion wheats that com- bine the desirable qualities and which lack those disadvantageous to the grower and the baker. Sometimes a very sim- ple recombination is of very great commercial value (Fig. 52). The so- called Havana type of Fic. 52.—Nicoliana tabacum variety Wrapper tobacco grown “Havana.” A stocky habit of growth with in the Connecticut about 20 large leaves. ‘ River valley has large leaves and a short stocky habit of growth. It produces 18-21 leaves. There is another type from Sumatra which has tall habit of growth with about 26 comparatively small leaves. These two types were crossed by Shamel. From this cross a new type called the Halladay has been A pplication of Biological Principles to Plant Breeding 125 produced having the greater number of leaves of the Suma- tra parent and the stocky habit of growth and large leaves of the Havana parent (Fig. 53). The first interpretation of this result was that an entirely new variation had appeared for the Sumatra does not usually have as many as 26 leaves. The writer has been able to show, how- ever, that the actual strain of Sumatra used as the parent had an average of 26 leaves, and data have now been collected which indicate that the new variety is a simple recom- bination of the characters possessed by the two par- ents giving a strain averag- ing 30-50 per cent greater yield than the old Havana variety (Fig. 54). In a similar way Orton has combined the edible quality of the watermelon with the resistance to wilt of Fic. 53.—Nicotiana tabacum variety the citron or stock melon; “Sumatra.” A tall habit of growth with Webber has combined the “"M* 7° small aves fine, long, strong lint of the sea-island cotton with the large bolls and productiveness of the upland cotton; Price has made many new combinations in tomatoes; and von Riimker has produced numerous valuable varieties of rye and barley. So the list might run on and on. Hundreds 120 Heredity and Eugenics of plant breeders are using these methods to produce thou- sands of new types annually. Most of them are worthless, nearly all of them are no better than what are already in commercial use, but the comparatively few that are Fic. 54.—Hybrid combining the desirable qualities of the “Havana” and “Sumatra”? varieties. really superior repay the time and money spent a thousand fold. I might add that it is the community at large that is highly repaid, however, for the plant breeder, unlike the inventor, never gets rich through his productions. A pplication of Biological Principles to Plant Breeding 127 Recently, accurately controlled investigations have shown that a strict Mendelian notation will interpret results that hitherto had been given the name blended inheritance. For instance, one may cross an eggplant, Solanum melon- gena, bearing large fruits with one bearing small fruits. In the first hybrid generation, fruits intermediate in size are produced. Segregation in the second hybrid generation is such that plants bearing fruit like either parent can be obtained if a large number of individuals (several thousand) are grown. Yet among the F, progeny, intermediates still occur in large numbers, and from them pure types can be secured. Most of the characters hitherto described are qualitative in nature. They are either present or absent in the differ- ent varieties. Such characters are generally dominant, in which case the heterozygotes are like the homozygotes in appearance. Other characters give heterozygotes inter- mediate in appearance, owing to incomplete dominance, but these intermediates can never be fixed. Owing to their heterozygous constitution they always segregate the paren- tal characters in the next generation. Size characters or quantitative characters, on the other hand, are often very complex. They are due to the interaction of many factors. For this reason blends may be obtained in the F, genera- tion that are homozygous for such a combination of game- tic factors that they always breed true to that condition (Fig. 55). Fortunately it is not necessary always to have plants that breed true to seed. Many commercial plants are propagated asexually by bulbs, tubers, cuttings, etc. Here one has a method of growing portions of a single plant for an indefinite length of time. Fruit trees, bush fruits, 128 Heredity and Eugenics strawberries, potatoes, pineapples, and many other kinds of economic plants belong to this category. This is a great advantage. One can so propagate homozygous strains if he wishes, but in addition he has a means of utilizing heterozy- gotes that would not breed true to seed and also of keeping the greater vigor that ac- companies heterozygosis. No better example of such work can be given than that of Webber on citrus fruits. The great bugbear of the Florida orange grower is the frost that occasionally comes, leaving devastation in its wake. Webber, therefore, set himself the definite problem of producing a frost-resisting orange. He made several reciprocal crosses between the com- mon orange and the hardy but worthless trifoliate Fic. 55.—Intermediate character of f eis an Fy hybrid. At left, Nicotiana panicu- OTaNnge (Citrus trifoliata). Jala; in center, hybrid; at right, Nicotiana Among the seedlings ob- dlala, : tained, several have proven valuable. They form a new class of citrus fruits and have been called Citranges. Three of these varieties have been named the Rusk, the Willits, and the Morton. The Rusk, which is a hybrid of orange crossed by ¢rifoliala, is a small fruit with a bitter tang like the pomelo. It makes excellent marmalade and preserves. The Willits, coming from a A pplication of Biological Principles to Plant Breeding 129 cross of orange upon /rifoliata, is a rough, but thin-skinned fruit, resembling an orange in appearance but a lemon in flavor. It is used as a condiment or for citrangeade. The Morton, coming from the same kind of cross as the Willits, is a large, juicy, almost seedless fruit, only slightly more bitter than the sweet orange. Young trees of these three varieties have endured a temperature of eight degrees above zero, and it is thought that by the use of them and of similarly obtained varieties, citrus fruit culture can be extended fully 400 miles north of the present region. In connection with this description of the production of new citrus fruits it may be well to mention that they are sometimes seedless. In fact, seedless fruits are often obtained by crossing. In true annuals reproducing by seed only, such productions would be of no value, for they would perish at the end of the first season. Seedless perennials, however, are among the most valuable horticultural varie- ties simply because they can be propagated asexually. In floral novelties, moreover, not only seedlessness but entire sterility is not a drawback to commercial worth, because sterile plants are often famous for their profuse flower clusters. It was stated earlier that one phenomenon of hybridi- zation was the production of fixed or constant first genera- tion hybrids. This statement was made from hearsay evidence. There are several cases in which either new characters or blended characters that breed true appear to have been formed, but they have not been studied with sufficient care for their mode of inheritance to have been accurately and finally decided. In crosses between cer- tain true species, hybrids have been produced that are 130 Heredity and Eugenics seemingly very constant and uniform. Perhaps the most famous of these are the blackberry-raspberry hybrids first produced by the late E. S. Carman and later by Luther Burbank and others. Several hybrids having a commer- cial value have been made in this genus (Rubus), and the small number of second generation progeny that have been grown are said to have bred approximately true. Prac- tically, it makes little difference about the exactness of this statement. One can simply say that for all ordinary intents and purposes, such hybrids breed true. To the scientist it makes a great deal of difference whether these hybrids are definite exceptions to the law of Mendel or not. The few data that we have are not sufficient to clear up this point, but several hypothetical explanations of the phe- nomena can be given that are in harmony with a belief in the universality of Mendelianism. Nothing is really known about segregation in these hybrids because the variations that occur are difficult to describe and because the plants have never been grown in large quantities. It is likely that numerous separately heritable characters are concerned in such crosses between true species, and when # pairs of character are concerned it takes four to the mth power seedlings to run an even chance that there will be one plant like each of the parents. When one considers that with ten pairs of characters, this means Over 1,000,000 individuals, he can see what enormous numbers are needed to give valid conclusions. Moreover, these hybrids are only partially fertile and some considera- tion must be given the possibility that selective fertilization among the gametes of the hybrid may occur. To take a hypothetical case, suppose two plants are crossed in which the flowers of one are twice the length of the flowers of the Application of Biological Principles to Plant Breeding 131 other and that the extra length of the longer flower is con- trolled by three or four separately heritable factors. If only a few of the egg cells and pollen cells can fuse on account of the dissimilarity of their gametic constitution, one would expect only those seeds to be formed that would result from the fusion of the germ cells nearest alike. Intermediates would therefore be more likely to occur than extremes. There is one other possible way of accounting for con- stant intermediate races. In crossing species of the genus Nicotiana, I have had plants develop from carefully guarded and supposedly hybrid seed that were exactly like the maternal plant. These seeds must have resulted from apogamy or polyembryony, that is, from the development of an immature egg cell without fertilization. The phe- nomenon was evidently induced by the extraordinary irritation of the foreign pollen. The question then arises: May not the difficulty of maturing sex cells in the F, genera- tion of a wide cross sometimes cause apogamous seed development and therefore a continued propagation of a constant and uniform race ? These pieces of work illustrate the various distinct types in the improvement of plants by hybridization. Intentionally, little has been said regarding the fixation of desired character combinations when the new varieties obtained are to be reproduced by seed. The reason for this omission is that the selective method used after hybridiza- tion is the same as that used upon crops whose small seeds and tendency to vary makes it difficult or unnecessary to produce artificial hybrids. The method is Vilmorin’s and is based upon the fact that one cannot tell the most productive or otherwise desirable plant by inspection. The true basis 129 Heredity and Eugenics of selection must be the average condition of the progeny of a plant determined by actual field tests. The entire object of selection is accomplished when a homozygous strain or strain genotypical for the desired qualities is isolated. The idea is simple; to put the idea into practice successfully is often a tedious and difficult task. As in hybridization, the ease with which results can be obtained by selection depends largely upon flower structure. In selection, however, the relative facility with which artificial cross-pollination can be accomplished is of small importance. What one wishes to know is whether cross- pollination or self-pollination takes place naturally. Practically all plants are occasionally cross-fertilized naturally, and many of them have devices whereby they are nearly always crossed; but, as we have already seen, though cross-fertilization is an advantage to a plant, it is not at all essential. Wheat, for example, is almost always self-fertilized; yet it has kept its vigor for thousands of years. The importance of this fact to the selectionist is readily seen. If seed from several varieties of wheat is mixed and planted, each variety remains true to its type because of self-pollination, and each strain can be recovered in one generation. In like manner, if desirable variations occur in a wheat variety, it is a simple process to sepa- rate them from the parent strain, for the two are mixed mechanically. It is only necessary to save seed from individual plants and grow them in separate rows or plots. One can see immediately whether the desirable variation is inherited or not, and if so the thing is done. In a cross-pollinated plant the method is the same, but the work is not so easy. The pollen is carried through long distances by the wind or by insects, and even with carefully Application of Biological Principles to Plant Breeding 133 isolated plots the plants are often intercrossed. Each prize plant selected for future breeding will have had a few and possibly many of its ovules fertilized by pollen from less desirable strains. When these seeds are grown they of course again fertilize the ovules of the desirable plants with a frequency proportionate to their number. In certain plants the process may be shortened by having recourse to artificial self-pollination. But unfortunately this cannot always be done. Suppose one were dealing with red clover where the flowers are small, almost sterile with their own pollen and produce only one seed. Im such a crop a long and tedious method of continuous selection must be used for there is no other way. One must simply keep in mind the supporting principle of all selection work, that the seeds of single plants are grown in isolated plots and the character of the mother plant is judged by the characters of the progeny. We have already seen from Mendelianism and the geno- type conception of heredity why this method is the only proper one, but perhaps an illustration will show the matter more clearly. The older method of selection, called variously the German or ‘‘mass selection” method by plant breeders and the “performance record”? method by stock- breeders, is based entirely upon the appearance or general character of the mother. For example, the German sugar beet raisers have for years analyzed large numbers of sugar beets and have grown their seed from the mother beets showing the highest percentage of sugar. No par- ticular attention was paid to planting from “blood lines” of high sugar content; those beets were bred from which appeared to be the best by their performance record in the polariscope test. A great many of these selected mothers 134 Heredity and Eugenics were simply high extremes belonging to “blood lines”’ that were low in their average sugar content. These individuals crossed with those from better ‘‘blood lines” and progress was made very slow indeed. In this short discussion on selection the writer has endeavored to make clear two points that may be sum- marized as follows. Plants are exceedingly variable but the majority of these variations are simply accelerations or retardations of the development of the whole or of cer- tain parts of the plant due to good or bad environment at critical stages of the plant’s growth. These variations are not inherited because the reproductive or germ cells are not affected. Other variations, however, are being constantly produced by nature—though much more rarely—which do effect the reproductive cells and are transmitted to the plant’s progeny. These variations are the basis of selec- tion. They are constant from the beginning—although their possible presence in a heterozygous condition may make it seem otherwise—and remain so unless changed by a second variation affecting the same constituent in the reproductive cells that is due to develop the character in question. The second point to be emphasized is that the whole aim and action of selection is to detect the desired heritable variants among the useful commercial plants and through them to isolate a race with the desired char- acters. When such a homozygous race is produced, selec- tion can then do nothing until nature steps in and produces another desirable variation. The progeny test is the way to accomplish this end. It does this by showing us to which strains each mother plant belongs. It is a sure test whether the heterozygous condition is simple or complex. If it is a question of which seeds of a maize ear are homozygous and A pplication of Biological Principles to Plant Breeding 145 which are heterozygous for starchiness the matter is cleared up at once. If it is a question of which of a lot of beans is homozygous for the gene-complex of large size, the com- plexity of factors concerned may require a greater number of progeny, but the test is valid in the end. There remains for mention a phenomenon of some inter- est even though it has produced few varieties of plants of commercial importance. This is the sudden appearance of a branch with characteristics different from the mother plant upon which it is borne that may be cut off and propa- gated asexually. It is the so-called bud sport. It is of practically no importance outside of the production of floral novelties. Perhaps this is accounted for by the fact that bud variations nearly always affect the same characters that have previously been changed in the same way through seed variations. Furthermore, the change is practically always the loss of a character which leaves little oppor- tunity for the production of the real novelties through progressive variations (Fig. 56). The production of the smooth-skinned peach, the nec- tarine, as a sport, from the ordinary peach tree, is the classi- cal example of this type of variation. This is undoubtedly simply the loss of the Mendelian factor for presence of the down upon the fruit, and might be expected to come about through some abnormal cell division in much the same man- ner that variations occur in the reproductive cells. They are usually not inherited through the seeds. This is what would be expected. It is said, however, that sometimes such variations come perfectly true to seed. If this is so, one must suppose that the varying plant cell was one which could give rise to the reproductive cells. Since nothing definite is known of this matter, however, speculation does 136 Heredity and Eugenics not seem wise. It is simply mentioned as one other way in which new plant varieties originate. In conclusion I wish to anticipate a possible question. ic. 56.—Frond of Boston fern and types that have arisen through bud varlation. What help have the new biological principles been to the commercial plant breeder? A prominent horticulturist has said that the new discoveries have made necessary no changes in method. With the exception that they have A pplication of Biological Principles to Plant Breeding 137 shown the scientific basis for Vilmorin’s method of selection, which before was not in general use, the statement is largely true. Yet they have been of inestimable practical value. They are time savers. In hybridization, one no longer grows large quantities of first generation hybrids and small quantities of second generation hybrids, for he knows that it is in the second generation that the desirable recom- binations of characters will occur. In many cases he can even predict with some accuracy the exact number of second generation individuals which it will be necessary for him to grow to obtain the desired result. And even when this cannot be done he knows that the blended characters of the first hybrid generation do not mean that he has failed to attain his object. It is simply a matter of growing large numbers in the second hybrid generation that insures success. Furthermore, the plant breeder has a means at hand to show what characters are heterozygous and there- fore unfixable. He therefore no longer wastes time in striving for a pure strain of a heterozygous type such as the Blue Andalusian fowl. Nor does he still regard the appear- ance of “rogue” plants in his nursery beds as a necessary affliction of Providence. He has learned that they are simply recessive segregates and can be prevented by prop- erly protected hand-pollinations. In the field of selection the new ideas are still more economical of time. To the belief that faith and continu- ous selection toward an ideal would produce any desired result has succeeded the idea that nature alone produces variations and that man’s duty is to be alert to grasp their possibilities and to make the most of them. No longer is it believed that many generations of work are necessary to purify a commercial variety of plants from undesirable 138 Heredity and Eugenics characters. No longer is there belief that the results of selection are continuous, that it gradually perfects a char- acter. We work for strains homozygous for characters that we know are there, and, by our direct methods we get them without loss of time. WILLIAM LAWRENCE TOWER Associate Professor of Zodlogy, the University of Chicago CHAPTER VII RECENT ADVANCES AND THE PRESENT STATE OF KNOWLEDGE CONCERNING THE MODIFICATION OF THE GERMINAL CONSTITUTION OF ORGANISMS BY EXPERIMENTAL PROCESSES INTRODUCTION Through inheritance there comes the rhythmic repeti- tion in each specific organic form of a precise and definitely repeated series of events ending in the production of an individual which in time sets free from itself a highly organ- ized and specific mass—a gamete, which, when properly combined with some other gamete and nurtured, repeats again the series of events which took place in the parent bodies which preceded them. Throughout all of this com- plex, rhythmic process a material basis, the germ plasm, keeps intact from parent to progeny genetic lines of descent. This continuity of material basis—first clearly recognized by Gustav Jaeger, and later woven by Weismann into his germ-plasm theories—while known in its gross appear- ance and many of its behaviors, is nevertheless quite unknown as regards its real constitution and the means through which it does reproduce so accurately in each generation the sequence of events characteristic of its specific organic form. The purely a-priori hypotheses of the “constitution” of this substance in reality help us little or not at all as a basis for experimental investigation. The id-determinant- biophore fabric of Weismann, Naegeli’s micellae chains, 141 142 Heredity and Eugenics DeVries’ pangene complex are no better and perhaps no worse than the hypothesis that racial memory is the basis of inheritance, or that the complex or harmonious equi- potential systems of Driesch “explain” the phenomena. As far as the facts of development and heredity are concerned, they might go on indefinitely repeating any given series of events, but there would be only. one type of organism, alike at any and all points in the genetic chain. Adequate evidence that there have been changes in this series of events in the past and that changes are now going on is found in the array of specific organic forms that exist and have existed through geological history. How have these changes been produced ? The nineteenth-century biology formulated its hypothe- ses around two widely different concepts; an extreme transmission hypothesis in which modifications arising in peripheral parts—soma, i.e., personal peculiarities developed in life—are transmitted to the progeny, through being in some manner incorporated into the germinal constitution of the race; and the hypothesis that changes in the race arise primarily in the germinal substance itself and appear later in the soma. The idea of the peripheral origin of variations through the stress of the conditions of life dates back to Buffon, Erasmus Darwin, and Lamarck, whose ignorance of things now common knowledge had led them to express the opinion, backed by much circumstantial evidence, that the condi- tions of life, especially when changed, produced variations which were heritable in the race. Darwin advanced the hypothesis of an atomistic mechanism whereby this trans- mission could conceivably be produced and upon this pro- visional hypothesis of Pangenesis have been based _ all Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 143 subsequent hypotheses of the peripheral origin of modifi- cations and of their inheritance. The hypothesis of the peripheral origin and transmission of variations is shown in diagram in Fig. 57A, where the cause of variation (x) impinges upon the organism and External cause of somatic modification pate Development Adult PARENTS PROGENY Fic. 57.—(A), Diagram to represent Darwin’s Provisional Hypothesis of Pangenesis and current neo-Lamarckian conceptions and (B), the continuity of the Germ Plasm Hypothesis of Weismann. induces a change, variation (y), and from the cells composing this modified part, as from all other cells of the body, gemmules, minute masses of matter, electrolites, or some- thing, are thrown off and these units conserve the power of reproducing the replica of the part from which they come, either normal or modified. These gemmules are supposed to be gathered in the gametes, and in reproduction are supposed 144 Heredity and Eugenics to be redistributed peripherally and to reproduce the dupli- cate of the particular character from which they arose. If now (x) impinging upon the organism, gives (y) a new variation, then new sorts of gemmules are supposed to be formed, and these on being gathered up and carried along in reproduction by the gametes will cause to reappear in the progeny the modified character. Repeated impact of (x) may, in the opinion of the adherents of the view, succes- sively increase (y). All theories of the peripheral origin and inheritance of variations are patterned after Darwin’s hypothesis, and although they have different expressions or terms for the carriers of the variation: nerve force, force, ions, electrolites, energy, etc., they are in essence the same conception and are all operated by the same mechanism. Radically opposed to this theory of the peripheral origin of variation is that of the central or germinal origin of varia- tion, in which the cause («) acts upon the germ and pro- duces the change in the germinal constitution which, when the germ undergoes development, produces the divergent character (y), the variation (Fig. 57B). Much logic has been expended upon this problem of transmission. Weismann has made a masterly analysis of the situation and can discover no reason for any con- clusion other than in favor of the germinal or central origin of all variations that are efficient in evolution. Spencer, Cope, Eimer, Semon, Rignano, and others have tried to equal Weismann’s logical analysis of the problem, but without any conspicuous success. The problem is one for experiment and not for solution by logic. Very tiresome are the multitude of arguments, and the arrays of “plausible instances,” and of the “facts” which “can only be explained” thus and so. Many times has the Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms — 145 whole field been gone over and summarized, and yet one sees no progress, nor can progress be expected from this method of attack. Only precise experimental procedure can in any way aid in the solution of the problem, and all too often the experiments to test these theories, especially the earlier ones, admit of diverse interpretations, so that they are in the main inconclusive. In this chapter are presented some of the data accumu- lated in recent efforts to gain precise experimental knowl- edge of how germinal changes are brought about. I shall, therefore, present in two divisions the data and conclusions bearing upon the two supposed methods of change, with such brief discussion and correlation of the already over- discussed and over-correlated literature as may be necessary. THE TRANSMISSION OF SOMATIC VARIATIONS It has been proved that variations do arise primarily in the germinal substance, and appear secondarily in the soma; but can it be proved that modifications arising in the soma, are transmitted to and incorporated into the germinal constitution and appear in subsequent generations? It is apparent that properly planned and conducted experiments are alone of service in the attempt to solve this question. It in no wise strengthens the position of the supporters of the theory of the peripheral origin of variations to present an extensive array of examples not explicable excepting through the use of this idea, and even though it “does explain” and ‘‘may explain” a huge array, or even all of the problems of evolution, it does not thereby become a proven truth. Special creation equally well explains all the phenomena, if certain assumptions be accepted as true. However, not until this or any other form of transmis- 140 Heredity and Eugenics sion can be obtained in properly guarded experiments and reproduced at will, can the process be admitted as a true evolutionary process. The problem, therefore, is to produce “‘somatic variations” ina soma at such a time, or in such a fashion, that the germ cells will not be affected by the action of the incident forces used, and then by breeding discover if the change appears in the progeny arising from the unstimulated germs. Evi- dence of somatic influence upon germinal material may also be obtained by transplanting germ glands, especially ovaries, into different somas, as has been done by several experi- menters.* The recent experiments of Guthrie,’ Castle,’ and Daven- port? are well adapted to showing any possible action of the soma upon the germ. In Guthrie’s experiments proper care was apparently not taken to determine the character of the stocks used and to preclude the possibility of regener- ated ovaries. Therefore his results are not conclusive. Guthrie describes his experiments in the ingrafting of ovaries between young females of single-combed black and single- combed white leghorns, as follows: During the summer of 1904 I exchanged the ovaries between two black and two white leghorn pullets, weighing about 650 gms. each. One black and one white pullet were saved for controls. All did well for some time after the operations, but during the winter, before the laying season began, their condition became extremely poor, owing largely to being kept in inappropriate quarters. ‘Castle has recently given a comprehensive résumé of the ingrafting of germ glands to which reference should be made for more detailed consideration. W. E. Castle and J. C. Phillips, On Germinal Transplantation in Verlebrates, Carnegie Institution of Washington, No. 144, 1911. 2>C. G. Guthrie, “Further Results of Transplantation of Ovaries in Chickens,” Jour. Exp. Zool., V (1908). 3 [bid., 1911. 4C. B. Davenport, Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. and Med., VIL (1910), 168. Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 147 On August 25, 1906, another series of pullets of the same strains were similarly operated upon, controls being saved as before. They weighed about 750 gms. each, the white ones being slightly the heavier. All did well after the operation. No marked differences in egg produc- tion were found between the control and operated hens, nor in the fertility of the eggs. The operated hens at the beginning of the laying season were somewhat lighter than the controls. In other respects no differences were observed in either the hens, eggs, or chicks. The eggs became fertile in two to four days after mating and on cessation of mating the eggs became infertile in eleven to nineteen days, the majority becoming so on the fifteenth day. Control hens B, and W,) mated to the rooster of the same breed gave uniformly black fetuses and chicks in the case of the black hen, and white fetuses and chicks in the case of the white hen. The normal black chicks had grayish-yellow breasts and throats and frequently the under surface of the tops of the wings was light colored as well, but the plumage of the entire dorsal surface was always solid black. The light-colored areas on the ventral surface were uniformly black after the first moult. Occasionally a normal black may retain one or several white feathers in the tip of the wing per- manently, but this is of rare occurrence and such white feathers have not been observed in any other situation. The normal white chicks were pure white to light buff when hatched, but after the first moult they were always pure white. The black hen (B,), carrying an ovary from a white hen (W.) mated to the white rooster, gave about equal numbers of white and spotted fetuses and chicks. (In all cases of very small white fetuses, spots may have been overlooked.) The white hen (W.), carrying an ovary from a black hen (B,) mated to the white rooster, gave white, black, and spotted fetuses and chicks. The spotted ones outnumbered the others combined. The black hen (B.), carrying an ovary from a white hen (W.) mated to the black rooster, gave ordinary black, and black fetuses and chicks with white legs, in about equal numbers. In regard to the chicks from this hen described as ordinary black, some doubt exists as to whether the ventral light-colored area described for normal black chicks was not lighter and greater in extent in all cases than in the normal chicks, 148 Heredity and Eugenics The white hen (W.), carrying an ovary from a black hen (B,) mated to the black rooster, gave uniformly spotted chicks, i.e., white chicks, with black spots on the dorsal surface of the head, neck, wings, back, or on the tail. Owing to the uniform results from the controls, it may be assumed that the strains of chickens used breed true to color. Therefore any variations in the offspring of the operated hens were due to other influences. The fact that in all cases of the operated hens, white or black or spotted fetuses or chicks were produced (i.e., the offspring showed variations from the normal in color markings) shows: 1. That the eggs from each of the operated hens were from the transplanted ovary. Take hens B, and W2. These hens were bred to the roosters of their color. Had some portion of their own ovary not been removed at the time of the operation (a remote possibility) and was functioning, then we would have expected solid offspring like the controls. But such was not the case. In the offspring from B, in which the male and foster mother were black, black predominated but white occurred. This must have come through the white ovary. In the offspring of W2, in which the male and foster mother were white, white was the predominating color but black occurred. The black therefore must have come from the black ovary. If we accept the statement that in ordinary crossing of black and white breeds the white is dominant, then we assume that the same is not true for this kind of (female) crossing, or that the original color influence was more strongly preserved in the black than in the white ovary. From the constancy in the results in the above two hens, we may conclude that the ovaries transplanted into the other two hens, B, and W,, were the ones functioning during the laying season also. 2. The foster mother exerted an influence on the color of the offspring. Take hens B, and W;. These hens were bred to the rooster of the opposite color, i.e., the color of the transplanted ovary. Yet in the former the majority, and in the latter all of the offspring were spotted, i.e., white with black spots on the dorsal surfaces. In B, the male and ovary were white and the foster mother black; in W, the male and ovary were black and the foster mother white. In both cases white predominated in the offspring. It would seem, therefore, Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 149 if we leave the question of dominance out of account, that the foster influence of the white hen was stronger than that of the black hen. _ If, on the other hand, we consider the foster influence equal in both cases, then we can explain the results as due to the dominance of the white in the male or ovary. Guthrie’s contention is that the ovaries were wholly removed, and did not regenerate; that the ingrafted ovaries developed, functioned, and were influenced by the foster soma to produce changes in gametic constitution. The doubtful points are concerning the nature of the stock and its gametic make-up, which was not tested in adequate manner, and the gametic constitution of all fowls is known to be very complex (cf. Bateson, Saunders, Davenport, and others); and the possibility of regenerated ovaries. Daven- port has repeated the experiments on other but well-known stocks, and summarizes his findings as follows: To test these experiments [Guthrie’s] I transplanted ovaries from a cinnamon-colored, heavy-boot, pea-combed, low-nostriled hen which breeds true to a white, non-boot, V-combed, five-toed, high-nostriled hen, and mated her with a cock whose characters resembled those of the hen from which the eggs had been borrowed. Had the engrafted ovary been functional, the chicks must have all been like the cock. Actually, they were exactly what expectation calls for when such a cock is mated to such a hen like the so-called foster mother. The engrafted eggs are not functional; the ovary had degenerated. Six experiments of this sort were made altogether and in no case was there evidence of a functional graft; far less of an influence on the eggs of the foster mother’s soma. These experiments, made on better-known stock, with many sources of error constantly in mind, give exactly con- tradictory results. Equally convincing and identical in result are the investigations of Castle in guinea-pigs, where no effect of the foster soma upon the ingrafted ovary was 150 Heredity and Eugenics found. Castle has made extensive transplantations of ovaries, especially in guinea-pigs, and finds that: Out of seventy-four cases six died as the immediate result of the operation; four of these were cases in which a ventral incision was tried. Summarizing the results of his operations he finds that in the results of the entire series only one grafted animal had young from her grafted tissue; grafted ovaries functioned in six other cases, but did not produce young. Ten animals regenerated their own ovaries, and three of these had young. Forty-two showed post-mortem com- plete atrophy of the genital tract and absence of ovarian tissue. The remainder comprises fifteen cases in which results were not fully deter- mined. On January 6, 1909, the left ovary was removed from an albino guinea-pig (Fig. 58B), then about 5 months old, and the ovary of a pure black guinea-pig about a month old (Fig. 584) was fastened near the tip of the uterine horn, distant a centimeter or more from the site of the ovary removed. One week later, January 13, a second operation was performed, in which the right ovary of the albino was removed, and as a graft was introduced the ovary of a second young black guinea-pig of like age with the first but of different ancestry. After the albino had fully recovered from the second operation she was placed with an albino male (Fig. 58C), with which she remained until her death about a year later. On the 23d of July, 198 days after the first operation, she gave birth to two female young. One was black, but bore a few red hairs. A photograph of this animal at the age of two or three months is shown in Fig. 58D. The other young one was likewise black, but had some red upon it, and its right forefoot was white (Fig. 58£). On October 15 the grafted albino bore a third young one, a male, which, like those previously born, had a few red hairs interspersed with black. A photograph of this animal is shown in Fig. 58P. On January 11, 1910, the grafted albino was observed to be pregnant for the third time, and this time she was very large. Unfortunately, on February 2, she died of pneumonia with three full-grown male young im utero. The skins of these animals were saved and a photo- graph of them is shown in Fig. 58G, H, and A. Like the other three Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms — 151 YOUNG 10] PURE BLACK LINE AND SOURCE OF TRANSPLANTED OVARIES IN g No 27 QUNE SO LINE ALBINO ALBINO Cc 9 27 crossep 6654 AND REMAINED TOGETHER UNTIL DEATH OF 9 3 No.654 K DIED AND THEIR BLACK COAT- ED FOETUSES WERE REMOVED FROM HER UTERUS Fic. 58.—To show the lack of effect of foster soma upon introduced ova (Modified from Castle.) See text for further discussion of this figure. 152 Heredity and Eugenics young they were black, but with a few red hairs among the black ones. They bore no white hairs. An autopsy made an hour after the death of the mother showed on the left side a distinct ovarian mass about a centimeter from the coiled part of the oviduct; that is, approximately the position where the graft from the pure black guinea-pig was fastened at the first operation. On the right side the mesentery of the oviduct was adherent to the body wall where an incision had been made at the second operation, and a small amount of tissue, regarded as possibly ovarian, was there observed. ‘The tissue from the left side was found to contain numerous large egg follicles, some already well advanced, containing a lymph space; in addition a number of corpora lutea were observed. On the right was found a small amount of undoubted ovarian tissue, with one well-advanced egg follicle, but the whole apparently was strongly encapsuated, so that no eggs could be discharged even if they came to maturity. It is interesting to note that both grafts persisted, though taken from different animals and transferred at different times. This result suggests a possible susceptibility on the part of the animal grafted. Female 1,970, a daughter of the grafted albino, was mated with the albino male, her father, and bore three young, two of which were albinos and one black with some red hairs. If female 1,970 had been the daughter of a pure black mother, instead of a grafted albino, we should have expected her to produce an equality of black and albino young. The observed result was the nearest possible numerical agreement with this expectation. A control mating of the albino male was made with a female of pure black stock. As a result there were produced two litters of young, including five individuals, all black, but with red hairs interspersed. This result shows that the red hairs found on the six young of the grafted albino, were due, not to foster-mother influence of the grafted albino, but to influence of the male parent. The young of the grafted mother were exactly in color such as the black guinea-pig which furnished the graft herself might have been expected to bear had she been mated with male 654 instead of being sacrificed to furnish the graft. The white foot borne by one of the young forms no exception to this statement. Spotting characterized the race of guinea-pigs from Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 153 which the father came. He himself was born in a litter which con- tained spotted young, whereas neither the pure-bred black race that furnished the graft, nor the albino race that received it was character- ized by spotting. Inasmuch as the offspring of albino parents are invariably albinos, it is certain that the six pigmented offspring of the grafted female were all derived from ova furnished by the introduced ovarian tissue taken from a black guinea-pig. This tissue was introduced while the con- tained ova were still immature, and it persisted in its new environ- ment for nearly a year before the eggs were liberated which produced the last litter of three young. These young, like the earlier litters, gave no indication of foster-mother influence in their coloration. The conclusion is forced upon us that the egg cell during its growth does not change in germinal constitution. Its growth is like the growth of a parasite or of a wholly independent organism; what it takes up serves as food; this is not incorporated merely in the growing organism; it is made over into the same kind of living substance as composes the assimilating organism. In all of these transplanted germ glands, it is true, as Castle recognizes, that his evidence and that of others does not disprove the possibility of foster-soma influence, but it is certain that the evidence is at present entirely against such influence. There are, however, two possibilities present in these experiments which should be kept clearly in view: 1. The transmission of some character from the foster soma to the germ and its incorporation therein. 2. The power of the foster soma to produce new surround- ings as a result of the transplantation, thereby arousing new physical or chemical activities incident upon the trans- planted germ cells. If the first effect occurs as a result of the transplantation, it is to be expected and must be proven that some essentially entire characters are introduced from the foster soma into the alien germ cell and this seems in all critical experiments 154 Heredity and Eugenics (Castle’s, Davenport’s) not to have taken place. As for Guthrie’s experiments with poultry, it seems as if Daven- port’s adequately controlled and carefully repeated experi- ments gave results which show very clearly that Guthrie’s cases are due to regenerated germ glands and impure stocks, and not to foster-soma influence. The second possibility, however, is a far different one and would show only varia- tions of gametic constitution of the alien germs and not intro- duced characters. In other words, there is this fundamental difference between the two possibilities: the foster soma may act merely as a new environmental complex providing new physical and chemical states which may modify the physiological activities of the ingrafted germ cells; and this is very different from the conception of the foster soma as formulating ‘‘a something” bearer of its characters or character, which ‘‘something”’ it transmits to the germ, which ‘‘something” then reproduces in development the replica of the somatic part or character from which it came. Neo-Lamarckians reply to the results obtained from these grafting experiments by the statement that the trans- planting of ovaries is highly abnormal and would not occur in nature, and would not be repeated in sequences long enough to get the kinetic effect necessary to induce germinal change, and therefore the experiments in no wise satisfy the require- ments of their hypothesis. The idea of repeated impacts producing an accumulated kinetic effect only after long periods of activity finds most complete expression in the curious theories of Rignano,’ which, however, are only another logical subterfuge to EB. Rignano, Sur la transmissibililé des caractéres aqguis. Paris, 1906. Also § ce) transl., Open Court Pub. Co., 1911. Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 155 maintain the cherished dogma of the biogenetic repetition of ontogenetic stages and inheritance through a transmission of some kind. Adequate answer to these hypotheses would seem to exist in the non-inheritance of modifications of nose, ear, lips, etc., of many savage tribes, often repeated with in- tense kinetic effects of pain and stimulation, or of the feet of Chinese women, bandaged and modified through long series of generations, but these to the earnest neo-Lamarckian are mutilations and of course are not to be expected to be inherited. Curiously enough the “idea” only ‘‘works”’ in those instances where there are no facts or evidences available for analytical investigation. Distinctly different from the results of grafting experi- ments or the arguments from plausible interpretations of past series of phylogenetic states, are the interpretations placed upon many experimental series not properly guarded. Thus Semon’s interpretation of the results of many experi- mental series is justified from his point of view because so many investigations have not been sufficiently critical in orientation, nor in analytical procedure. Thus, for example, the experiments of Standfuss, Fischer, Pictet, Woltereck, Kammerer, Pshibram, Zederbauer, and others admit of interpretations from either point of view. What is unques- tionably shown is change in gametic constitution, permanent and heritable, but not capable of answering the fundamental questions involved. That organisms may be modified by incident conditions there is no reasonable doubt, but the question is, how? If the discovery of the methods of change is desired, then experiments made upon known materials under carefully guarded conditions are necessary, and are our only means of obtaining real knowledge of the underlying processes. 156 Heredity and Eugenics It is obvious that progress in the solution of the problem can be made only through experiments based upon known materials which must meet certain rigorous requirements. The experiments of many observers with plants and animals show clearly that changes are produced which are inherit- able in following generations, but do not produce accurate data upon critical theoretical points. Thus, for example, Sumner’s recent work on mice is entirely of this order and gives only unreliable results. Nor can experiments give true data upon these points unless the following conditions are complied with: t. A stock of known character, whose behavior, germinal constitution, variability, etc., have been determined for a series of generations and kept in strictest pedigreed line cultures. The stock for experiment must be clarified and reduced to a homogeneous condition as far as possible, and the presence of minor strains fully determined and eliminated. 2. It must be known what stages in the development of the germ cells, if any, are capable of being influenced, and how—by any force intended to be used later as a somatic modifier. Further, the behavior in inheritance of these germinal modifications, if any, must be known for several generations. 3. The somatic change must be induced at a time when the germ has been found to be not sensitive to the stimulus employed, so that opportunity may be provided whereby there will supposedly be accumulated in the modified soma that something, carrying the potentiality of reproducing the modifications which the soma has acquired and which are believed by many to become incorporated into the growing germ cells as part of their constitution. Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 157 4. In any experiments four parallel series must be carried: (a) a parent stock from which at the start is derived the experimental stock, (b) the controlled stock reared parallel to the parent stock but under controlled conditions, (c) the experimental stock, which is a line culture sub- jected to conditions of experiment and continued throughout under the conditions of experiment, but from which is taken at intervals, and (d) the test series, which are the progeny of modified stock returned to conditions of the control and parent, to test the constancy or reappearance of induced modifications. 5. All lines must be group cultures mated at random to obviate in the fullest possible manner any traces of selective effects; that is, to breed from pairs of selected extreme individuals might easily lead to the selective accumulation of germinal variations normal to the race, or the isolation of hitherto unrecognized pure lines, and thus give rise to false conclusions. If these conditions are complied with in any series of experiments, the results will be an accurate answer to the problem—no matter whether brief or long-continued action be necessary to bring about the inheritance—because this may also be tested if the soma be modified at a time when the germ is not sensitive, and if this be repeated generation after generation the results obtained become more and more certain in their value as evidence for one or the other side of the controversy. Furthermore, experimental procedure of this kind will at once give an answer to the question of the influence of the soma as an environment upon the germ cell. That is, by incident conditions it is easy to modify temporarily the physiological state of the soma and gain further knowledge 158 Heredity and Eugenics concerning the influence of altered body states in producing germinal variations. It will not do to dodge the issue as to experimental methods by the citation of experiments where these precau- tions have not been taken and say, What matters it, the end result is the same—a modification? True, a modification, inheritable, has resulted in so many series of experiments that there no longer are any doubts thereon. But that does not and cannot answer the important theoretical question because experiments have all too often not been properly oriented and guarded. In this there is a direct experimental proof of the main contention of Lamarck and C. Darwin, that incident conditions produce permanent modifications. Naturally, in order to be permanent, any departure from the normal must become a part of the germinal constitution—a process which Lamarck never attempted to explain, and of which C. Darwin offered only a formal explanation in his provisional hypothesis of pangenesis. I have attempted to obtain what I considered reliable data upon this mooted point in the inheritance of somatic modifications, and one example of the results obtained when the procedure outlined has been followed may be given. To determine whether coloration changes in the soma produced as the result of changed environmental conditions are inherited, increased, or dropped in successive generations. Conditions —Temperature on the average 6° C. and relative humid- ity 10 per cent above that in nature, with other conditions natural. These conditions were planned to produce melanic tendencies in variation. A pparatus.—Shown in diagram in Fig. 59. The experiments in this series were conducted in the years 1900 to rgo4, and were carried through ten lineal generations. The conditions of temperature and moisture were as follows. 159 Isms itution of Organ l Const ma rf Germi von O Modificat *ysaq dq} SI way Map “-JO] vay] ay] Ul pasn udoq oavY pur Ued ZwaY [IO IO ‘sed ‘OTIJDOJA JOY —*‘aulty Jo pouad IYUYSPUL UL IOJ panurjyUoD aq Pod puv AyfestuLYoatu paurezUTLUL A]ISB dJoA oINYSIOUL PUL oIN7VIOdUII} JURISUOD SURI SIYY Aq ‘osoqe oovds ne ay Ul pur Y UT los 94} YSnom} oMprioduo} UIAIS V Jv Ie JO MOF JULIsUOD B BUIATS snyy “YW Ul TL yeysouay} ay Aq poljorjuoo sae1Zap Jo Joqumu urejJed & pasivi ust} sea pur ‘uoTLINyes pu sInjeIoduts} UaAIs B WV FY qo}vay ay} OUT SIoY oy} wo passed We oyp ‘ompessdtusz Jurjzsuos v ye ydoy aro YOTYM—G—syo}]J JO Jos ay} J9A0 passed y o7ULUAMTIp sny} Iv oy. £9 3e payoryie dumd ysneyxe uv Aq Y Ul pouTezUOd Are oy} Jno Surmvip Aq poureyqo star sty, ‘omjsiou pure ainjrsoduis} usats v ye Te YA g aqnz ay? YBnory? poyddns sea gory ‘y mel oyy ut ydoy oso sopooq ey, “Sep}ooq ul SuONwoy!pow SHvuOs Sut—npul ur posn snzeivdde jo adAz ay} Moys 0} wriseiq—O$ ‘ory 160 Heredity and Eugenics TABLE I TEMPERATURE AND Humipity CONDITIONS 10 | Maxi- | Mini- | . Devia- 7 A.M vag, 1 PM|3 PeM.| BPM ay anuan |Average|tion from z | | Normal Temperature dry bulb | | awa ; | | Sas Sr | Diy MEMES oo cile' 19: | 22" |Si! 123 ao) 43 | 3% | ga ° In experiment 2 oS. || sn) 31 | 230| 40:| 19 | 28.4 —6 Percentage of relative | | | | ae | | humidity: | | “4 | Tn matures wages *Too | 65 | 50 | BS | "Poo | tec | 43. | 74 In experiment. ..... *too | 85 | 60 | 75 | *100 | 100 55 | 84 =I | * Dew. In this series of experiments 21 per cent died in the larval stage and 9 per cent in the pupal, while 7o per cent appeared as imagines with the proper color modifications. Throughout the whole series the greatest care was taken to prevent the conditions of experiment from having any possible influence upon the germ cells in their growth periods and during maturation and fertilization. This was accomplished by removing the adults to normal conditions during the period of germ- cell growth and fertilization, the fertilized eggs being returned as soon as laid to the conditions of experiment. ee ee A Experiment 275, Control Experiment 27a, Subjected Generations Kept in Normal Conditions to Conditions of apes ment ee are 27b 27a Dice os nae7b 27a 100 ieeereree 9270 27b yo DV iirigh tt nee 27b 27bt 27a 270 Wage ste 276 27bt 27a 27at Vib roca ee 270 270% 27a 27a" Vile haces 270 : 27a? 27a 27a" aa a NTE asceanatas tet 270 27a? 27a 27a" 27a» EXe a ranons 27b 27a? 27a 27a" 27a» Meisiins 27b 2702 27a 27a 27a" Fic. 60.—To show the different parts of an experiment, in which L decemlineala was subjected to conditions which would modify the soma without modifying the germ plasm. The generations underscored were subjected to the conditions of the experiment; those not underscored were kept in natural surroundings. Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 161 By this means the color changes induced by these experiments were known to be purely somatic modifications. Moreover, a control series, derived from the same parents, was kept under normal conditions as a check. During the series also several lots were taken from the experi- ments and placed for several generations in normal conditions, and were then returned to experiment, and likewise lots were taken from the control and placed in experiment; and subsequently returned to con- trol. In this way a complete check was kept on the experiments. In Fig. 60 are represented the generations experimented upon and the proceedings followed with each. The experiment was divided into two parts—the experiment proper (27a) and the control (27)). In 27a the beetles were subjected to the conditions of experiment during ten lineal generations, with results shown in Fig. 61. A maximum deviation in coloration was produced at once toward a melanic state from which there was no deviation either above or below in the succeeding generations. In the third genera- tion of 27a the progeny were divided into two lots of equal size, one of which was kept in the conditions of experimentation, and the other returned to natural conditions. This second lot, known as 27a', after being bred during four generations in normal surroundings, was further separated into two portions, one of which was still kept in normal con- ditions as 27a™, while the other was returned to the conditions of experimentation as 27a'?. When the beetles in 27a" were returned to normal surroundings, they at once resumed their natura! characters and did not deviate therefrom during the four generations of 27a and the three of 27a", or seVen in all. However, the effect upon 27a"? of being returned to the conditions of experiment was an immediate return to the maximum melanic tendency before observed. From the sixth generation in 27a another lot of beetles, 27a, were taken and reared in normal conditions, with the result that they also immedi- ately reverted to the parental condition, and the same was true of 2743 in the ninth generation. In experiment 274 there appears a slight oscillating variability which, however, is of no consequence. In the second generation 27) was likewise separated into two lots of equal size, one of which, 27, was retained as control, while the other, 270", was placed in the conditions of experimentation for four generations, and later in the seventh generation returned to control with 270. 162 Heredity and Eugenics The effect upon 275' was an immediate production of the maximum melanic condition, which was retained throughout the four generations of experimentation, and lost only when 270! was returned to control. In Experiment 27 there was no artificial selection, all imagines being allowed freedom to mate and breed as in nature; hence the only selective influences present were those exercised in the mating of the beetles and by the conditions of the experiment, which eliminated a small percentage. From the data of this experiment the following conclusions are derived: 1. A deviation in an environmental complex at once causes the polygon of somatic variation and the modal class to shift as far from the normal as it can go under the given condition, and keeps them there until there is a return to the normal environmental complex, when the somatic variations also at once return to their normal state. 2. The color variations employed in experiment, which are purely somatic, are the direct result of a response to changed environmental conditions, in terms of increased or decreased activity in pigmentation. They may change as rapidly, as frequently, and in as many directions as the conditions producing them change, and they have no influence whatsoever upon the coloration of succeeding generations. This experiment, one of the earliest in my series, illus- trates fairly well the results obtained in several others carried out with the same end in view. In all, the result has been a uniform one, and entirely against the idea of transmission of somatic modifications. I have had many instances when I thought at first that a somatic transmission had taken place, but in all, further analysis and repetition clearly showed some defect in experimentation; and in no case have I been able to duplicate one of these occurrences, much less obtain experimental proof of somatic transmission in a repetition of the experiment. It is unfortunate that so many of the published experi- mental investigations of this subject are open to diverse 163 »f Germinal Constitution of Organisms ication o Modi{ Modal class of parent generations “SUOTIPUOD [eUIOU 0} PauIMJaI Suloq UO [RUIOU 9Y} OF WINJaI ay} pur ‘uote}UaWIsId JO WOILOYIpou oq} 07 yodsar WLM Lz “dxy Jo suonsod yuosayip ay} Jo asuodsar ay} Surmoys wrssviq—'1g ‘oq x) wo o \ awe é e \ NG eres cla Vehl 5 NY -=4 —e—- = d — aor BLS | |_| Ne BLS BLS 7 —>— 91 vag Sb 64 eo | i Or 6 8 Z 9 g ¢ € Zz n —— “panuljuod aiam syuawiiadxa yoiym Sulunp suoijesauar wsIUgiy ws}Ue aI ~ SUOI}LURA BY} jo uolpaIG C 104 Heredity and Eugenics interpretation and are not elucidations of the problems. The main reason why this is so is that the materials that have been used and the methods of experimentation have not been properly guarded. Thus, for example, the recent results of Kammerer with various amphibians and Lacerta, Woltereck’s investigations upon Daphnia, Zederbauer’s experiments with Bursa, Sumner’s with mice, as well as all of the older experiments, admit of ‘‘interpretation” from either point of view. Thus Semon “interprets” all of these and many more besides as showing the strength of the neo- Lamarckian position at the present time. A neo-Darwinian could make an equally good case of the same data. At present the conclusive evidence from Castle’s trans- plantation of ovaries in guinea-pigs, Davenport’s negative results with poultry, and experiments like those with color in Leptinotarsa have all given exactly the expected result without qualifications. MacDougal, in discussing some of these problems, says: ‘‘The time has now arrived when the claimants for neo-Lamarckianism and all of its conclusions must show cause for its further consideration, or else allow it to drop from the position of being seriously taken as a method of evolutionary advance.” With this most biologists will at present agree, but unfortunately, from time to time, some careless experi- menter with more partisan enthusiasm than judgment or experimental acumen will come forward with conclusions derived from experiments wherein the most elementary essentials of genetic research are ignored and reassert the transmission of somatic changes. Present experimental evidence, where critical, clearly indicates the increasing doubtfulness of the validity of the hypothesis of somatic transmission. Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms — 165 THE DIRECT MODIFICATION OF THE GERM PLASM Since there are the best of reasons for the conclusion that there is conditioned in the germ plasm the basis which determines the presence and manifestations of organic characteristics which are permanent in the race and in evolu- tion; and in that there are equally good reasons for a tena- cious adherence to the idea that all variations which are productive of evolutionary changes arise primarily in the germ and appear secondarily in the soma, it follows that any and all methods whereby changes are produced in the germinal material are of paramount interest. Modifications in this germinal material are the basis of permanent depar- tures from the racial mean, and at present the methods of production and cause of germinal variations are of great practical interest and value as well as of theoretical impor- tance. Germinal variations have been suggested to arise by five main methods. The direct action of external forces was the first to be suggested and is generally admitted to be an effective cause in the production of germinal variations. This was a mode of modification suggested by Buffon and Erasmus Darwin, later elaborated by Lamarck, and made the basis of his theory of evolution without any consideration whatsoever as to whether the variations were somatic or germinal. A half-century later variations which were supposed to have originated through the action of external conditions pro- vided in the main the array of individual differences upon which Darwin founded his theory of the ‘origin of species,” by means of natural selection. : Arising from the work of Darwin, and accentuated by the neo-Darwinians, is the idea of the production of variations 106 Heredity and Eugenics through selection, but how selection is conceived to pro- duce these results depends very largely upon an endless array of unproven neo-Darwinian assumptions. Hybridization is known to be productive of germinal variations, and in domesticated organisms a considerable number of useful forms have thus arisen. Frequently, however, these commercial hybrids, appearing to be con- stant, are only first generation hybrids indefinitely per- petuated by cuttings, as are many kinds of oranges, apples, grapes, etc., and most of these, if allowed to reproduce sexually, would in subsequent generations break up into the component types out of which they were built. There is evidence, however, to warrant the assumption that hybridization does result in the development of permanent modifications which are new to the strain in which they arise, and which persist indefinitely. Moreover, hybridization is a potent means of creating new and diverse combinations of existing qualities and attributes, which may account for no small portion of the “‘species”’ in nature, as well as in domestication. To what extent hybridization is a source of germinal variations in nature is undetermined, and this condition is largely due to the persistence of the dog- matism that hybridization is of rare occurrence, is ab- horrent to species in nature, and is really a product of domestication and the supposed loss of specific integrity and chastity induced by man and cultivation. Statements of this kind, however, are entirely a-priori orthodox preju- dices without foundation in fact. Recent work, especially by botanists, shows a considerable and increasing array of hybridizations occurring in nature, for example, in violets, which exhibit an abundance of crossings, with many resulting hybrids. Moreover, this condition is by no means limited to Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 167 violets or plants, but is coming to be recognized as common in nature. The production of germinal variations occurs by combin- ing slightly different conditions of the same attribute in the zygote (fertilized egg). This process, amphimixis, commonly advanced by neo-Darwinians, has at present almost no evi- dence in support of the theory that departures beyond the normal range of variation can be produced thereby, but it is at least a conceivable method by which variations might well arise, and is open to direct experimental investigation. The origin and development of variations through the operation of orthogenesis, a name descriptive of a condition, but personified to represent the agencies productive of the condition observed. It is conceivable that changes started in one direction or another may continue in that direction on the basis of the operation of the law of inertia, in a uniform direction and at a uniform velocity until they reach limits imposed by the physical nature of the part or of the organism in which they arise, or until they reach a stage of development where they become of selective value, and may be accelerated, retarded, or turned in other directions. In all of these possible modes of origin of germinal variations two groups of factors are always involved: first, the physical constitution of the material, with its array of qualities, attributes, and conditions, which is always the genetic product of an immense series of antecedent stages; second, incident forces from without the germinal material. These two groups of factors sustain definite and fundamental relations to each other, and the effort to understand the relations between these two fundamental groups has stimulated much of the investigation of the last decade. The physical or gametic constitution is the constant, and 168 Heredity and Eugenics the external conditions are the variable in the complex, and elimination or understanding of the most obvious vari- able is the first step in the study of gametic constitution and modification. Concretely, then, what is the réle of external factors in the production of germinal variations ? Satisfactory evidence as to the rdéle of external factors can be obtained only through careful experiments. These may be either experiments under laboratory conditions, or ex- periments in nature, and, if possible, both should be carried on at the same time upon the same materials. A. The Idea of Sudden Transmutation in the Germinal Material Succeeding Darwin, there arose a group of followers—the neo-Darwinians. Possessing all the attributes of followers, unable to grasp the breadth of view of their master, and see- ing but a particular phase of his general teachings, they endeavored to raise that to undue prominence and make it a universal motive force in the evolution of organisms. The neo-Darwinians in the last quarter of the nineteenth century, under the leadership of such men as Weismann, created what Eimer has termed the “principle of omnipo- tent natural selection.” It was attempted to establish pur- poseful selection as the sole efficient cause of variation and evolution in organisms, and in the effort, Weismann went so far as to place the selective process, not in the outside world, but in a microcosm within the germ plasm, making it in every way incapable of investigation, impossible of observation, and all-inclusive. Few today attach much importance to Weismann’s ‘germinal selection,” and the Weismannian theory remains in biological history one of those curious ideas comparable Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 169 to the quadrille of the centrosomes. The last of the curious hypotheses developed by the neo-Darwinians is that weird phrase of biology which took its rise from the work of Bates, Miiller, and Trimen, and which has been developed into the present theory of mimicry. Its supporters would have us believe that much transmutation is based upon a mimetic principle aided by the subsidiary principle of recognition marks. Here utilitarian variation and purposeful selection run riot and produce in every case a definite end, a pro- tected form. Naturally, these curious and thoroughly uncritical ideas, current among the neo-Darwinians, are their own answer. At present the neo-Darwinian concepts offer nothing that is of use as a working hypothesis in the further investi- gation of evolution, nor any logical ground for observation and induction in nature. The neo-Darwinian situation, and, also, the neo-Lamarckian are in reality two of those common developments which arise in every line of human thought—intellectual culs-de-sac. More as a protest against the neo-Darwinian situation than for any other reason, there arose, simultaneously, in England, on the Continent, and in America, the modern saltationist school. Thoroughly bored with the repetition by the neo-Darwinians of the same old facts sung to the same old tune, Bateson in England, DeVries on the Conti- nent, and others, determined to find an outlet, and all, I think, obtained their original inspiration from the recog- nition by Darwin that in many species variations repeatedly occur which stand apart from the rest of the population, and frequently are prepotent when bred back to the parent stock. Further, Darwin in his Origin of Species and The Variations of Animals ant Plants under Domestication cites 170 Teredity and Eugenics many instances in which there is good reason for believing that domesticated varieties of pigeons, birds, and cattle, and many plants, have arisen by the sporting process. Most biologists have insisted upon retaining that cherished dogmatism of the seventeenth and eighteenth centuries that there must be discontinuity between species, and the idea seemed plausible that if discontinuity existed between species when they were finished, there was no a-priori reason why it could not have arisen at the start. Therefore, Bateson, in the latter part of the nineteenth century, gathered what data existed in the literature on sports and discontinuous variations, in the effort to find an outlet from the cul-de-sac into which neo-Darwinianism had led English biologists. At about the same time DeVries in Holland became convinced from a somewhat different point of view that a similar process must be operative in the production of species in nature. DeVries sought, therefore, to find in nature plants which exhibited the kind of variation that he conceived of as being the basis of transmutation, and finally discovered that Oenothera Lamarckiana seemed to be undergoing exactly the sort of process which he hoped to find. Others became convinced that there were possibilities in this direction, and as a result there is at present a well- developed school of saltationists whose central idea is that progressive and efficient steps in transmutation take place through sudden, steplike variations, producing, as DeVries asserts, something quite new each time. Directly associated with the development of this idea, and contributing much to its development, was the redis- covery of Mendel’s paper on the ‘Behavior of Hybrid Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 171 Peas,’ which gave impetus to investigations that strength- ened and extended the saltationist conception. It would be rash indeed to deny that there are “‘sports”’ in the Darwinian sense, and DeVries’ “mutations” are asserted by some to be but the same kind of variations with a new name, but the fact of the occurrence of sudden variations is established beyond doubt. However, next to nothing is known concerning the rise and behavior of these sports and the part they play in the transmutation of organisms in nature, and it may be wisest to suspend judg- ment as to the relative importance of saltation as a method ofevolution. The saltation conception, however, considered in its broadest sense, has decided advantages over the neo- Darwinian and neo-Lamarckian positions, because it is directly open to experimental study and does serve as a fairly logical and workable hypothesis for investigation. In its broadest aspects it is in no way like Weismann’s theory, although DeVries has endeavored to give it a position not unlike that of germinal selection by placing all essential processes in the hypothetical pangenes. Out of this situa- tion perhaps the greatest advance that has been produced is the revival of interest in bionomic investigations and the clearing of the mist from many questions, even though the questions have not been fully answered. Regardless of what the future may have in store, the saltationist school has rendered biology a very real and lasting service in arousing new enthusiasm for the experimental study of evolution problems and in breaking away from neo-Darwinianism, neo-Lamarckianism, and orthogenesis, whose deadly chants were slowly but surely lulling into complacent inactivity the greatest heritage from Darwin—the experimental study of evolution problems. L72 Heredity and Eugenics The saltationist school, aside from a considerable num- ber of clearly formulated questions, has very clearly raised the issue as to whether changes in the constitution of the germinal material are accomplished by the slow quantita- tive accumulation of useful variations, or take place by sudden steps, appearing with discontinuity in the end result. Much experience with this method of quantitative accumula- tion had given adequate reason to distrust it as a particularly potent means of inducing experimental change, and refuge not infrequently had been sought in the soul-satisfying myths of germinal selection, orthogenesis, isolation, growth force, bathmic force, and many other intricately contrived and all inclusive hypotheses, but all were found utterly useless for actual experimental investigation and analysis, such as is demanded in physical and chemical science. In seeking for an outlet from the culs-de-sac of evolu- tionary science as it existed in the last quarter of the nine- teenth century, DeVries concluded that change, per saltiume, was quite as lable to be a real method of transmutation and sought to put it to a test by seeking in nature for species of plants that were undergoing this kind of change if such existed. Darwin had already noted the frequent occurrence of large sudden departures in both plants and animals, but was of the opinion that both large, sudden, and small fluctuations were operative in transmutation phenomena. The discovery of O. Lamarckiana (Fig. 62) in an abandoned field near Hilversum in Holland, into which it had escaped from a near-by park, provided most favor- able material for DeVries’ further study. Here it grew in quantity with two apparently newly arisen derivative forms, O. laevifolia and O. brevistylis. Plants from this waste land were taken into the botanic gardens of Amsterdam Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms — 173 Fic. 62.—Oenothera Lamarckiana, the original type of plant used by DeVries in his experiments. This is the stock from Hilversum, from which arose in suc- cessive generations a series of new forms by sudden jumps. From DeVries. 174 Heredity and Eugenics and there gave rise to several new types during the years of DeVries’ observation. Some of these arose in the experiments but once, as for example, O. gigas (Fig. 63), a tall, robust form with large flower, the finest of all the new types. It appeared in 1895 and was one out of about 14,000 plants, but was not the only new type to appear in the crop of that year. Six others were found: O. albida, of which there were fifteen examples (Fig. 64); O. oblonga (Fig. 65), of which 176 specimens appeared; O. rubrinervis, eight specimens; O. scintillans (Fig. 66), one specimen. This year gave the greatest number of new forms of any, although other years (1896, 1897) gave all but the O. gigas. A good idea of the real differences existing between these derivative forms and the parent plants is given in Fig. 67, where the plants are shown growing side by side. In Fig. 68 is given in condensed form the line of descent and the appearance of the derivative forms from year to year. If all be granted that is claimed for the separateness of these types, and admitting also that they are absolutely constant in type and in heredity, there still remains one striking difference between these DeVriesian ‘‘mutations”’ and the ‘‘sports,” ‘‘saltations,”’ etc., of other writers: namely, the mutations occurred in numbers in every genera- tion for a considerable period, through several consecu- tive generations; the sports of Darwin appear but once, rarely, and not successively. Upon the curious findings in O. Lamarckiana, DeVries has built the hypothesis of a premutation period in which the germ plasm was elaborating new pangenes which, when the pangenes reached a certain point, broke out into visible manifestation as mutants, and he further supposed that after a time the new Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 175 Fic. 63.—Oenothera gigas, a mutant of Oenothera Lamarckiana. This form arose but once in DeVries’ cultures (in 1895), out of a culture of 14,000 seedlings. From it has arisen a strong race now cultivated in many gardens in Europe and America. 170 Heredity and Eugenics Pic. 64.—Oenothera albida. Another type which has arisen from Oenothera Lamuarckiana, occurring with considerable frequency in successive years. Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 177 Tic. 65.—Oenothera oblonga. A type which has arisen from Ocnothera Lamarckiana. 175 Heredity and Lugenics Fic. 66.—Oenothera scintillans. A rather rare mutant of Oenothera Lamarck- tana. Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 179 O. rubrin O. Lamarckiana illans O. sci O. givas O. hirtella O. nanella O. Lamarckiana Fic. 67.—A series showing Qenothera Lamarckiana and several of its mutants growing side by side, illustrating From DeVries. characteristic differences in height, habit, foliage, branching, and flowering of the different forms. 180 Heredity and Eugenics pangenes would cease to be produced, or at any rate that mutation would cease and the parent species go on as before. Genera- : O. rubri- O. Lamarck- O. scintil- tions O. gigas O. albida O. oblonga nervis tana O. nanella O. lata lans Saka Ga sea Bois sSeet see cock ses eee sase ke way aeceae aguas -+-----—4 Gen. 8 j i Too 5 1 6. hivoo-| 21. | 4 aero 1898 | 9 0 3700 11, a fi 29 3 | 1800 9 eed Piette Seiy ey oe Sei an ae Gen. ! 25_| 135 | 20 | gooo| 49 |142| 6 | i nae ee 15 {76 8 | 14000 60 | 75 | 4 1895 Gen. 3 1890-91 Gen. t 1886-87 Mutants and Number observed in O. Lamarck- Mutants and Number different years lana observed in different years The mu- tating stem form Fic. 68.—Diagram showing in condensed form the genealogy of the Oenothera Lamarckiana family and its various mutants during successive years. The num- bers under each type represent the number of new types observed in each year. As far as causes were concerned, DeVries had little to offer except the suggestion that the cause was ultimately probably an external one. Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 181 There is not the least doubt as to the behavior of O. Lamarckiana and the appearance of the ‘‘mutants,” and it appeared to many that there was a good chance of producing “mutating races” by external forces acting upon the germ of the parent race. The last decade has produced a deal of evidence that external forces can produce germinal changes, but these are in all instances immediate and final. New, divergent types, more or less separated from the parent, have appeared, but in none are there subsequent mutations. I had been at work upon this problem and had found and reared sports of Leptinotarsa decemlineata as early as 1893, and on the appearance of DeVries’ work I began in a systematic way to try to produce mutating races by the use of external forces. I have thus far positively failed to produce a mutating race by these agencies, although I have been able to get changes in profusion, some of which I shall describe later. In too1 I tried to produce a mutating race by crossing L. decemlineata, L. juncta, and L. pallida, with the idea that perhaps the interbreeding and combination of the chief characters of the three into a hybrid complex would produce a type which under changed conditions of growth and development, or of changed or adverse environment, would give the mutation behavior of O. Lamarckiana. The early experiments were just beginning to show promise of interesting results when they were brought to anend. As subsequent results have shown, this was a good working hypothesis but these first experiments would not have led to the results wanted. In 1902 Bateson suggested that the mutation phenomena in Oenothera was possibly akin to a Mendelian splitting of a hybrid type. 182 Heredity and Eugenics From the year 1904 onward I have been able to carry out a number of suggestive experiments in the further effort to produce a mutating race experimentally. EXPERIMENTS IN THE SYNTHESIS OF A MUTATING STEM RACE An extensive set of these experiments has been in progress for some years, some of which have now developed far enough to allow of rather definite statements. The method employed has been to take species derived from nature from some restricted locality, to keep close watch upon what goes on in this locality, and also to analyze the composition of the species from this locality by cultures in the laboratory. In this way, stocks of known character are obtained from experiment, and also natural stocks whose attributes are well known are developed in the type localities. In the experiments in synthesis either pedigreed stocks from the laboratory, or the stocks from nature, or both, are placed in nature upon their food plant in isolated localities, or in large cages, and allowed to breed as if the introduction were a natural one. In 1904, an isolated area of about an acre upon the southern slope of a barranca, near Cuernavaca, was planted with food plants, upon which both L. signaticollis and L. undecimlineata would feed. In July, 1904, this spot was stocked with a culture of 210 specimens of L. signaticollis, from a standard location about a mile and a half distant, and 354 specimens of L. undecimlineata, obtained at El Hule, on the banks of the Rio Papaloapan. The groups were equally divided between the sexes, were young and vigor- ous, immediately began breeding, and intercrossed freely. Under experimental conditions these forms cross freely in Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 183 both directions, but out of them no new characters come as the result of ordinary crossing. In the first generation of this colony there was an abundance of individuals of both sexes of the signaticollis type, and of the wndecimlineata type, and of a highly variable intermediate hybrid type. A census was made of the population on August 14 to 17, with the following results: Signaticollis Type Mid-Type Undecimlineata Type 4,518 11,744 5,001 In this experiment, it was, of course, impossible to tell from inspection whether the signaticollis individuals were pure signaticollis, or pure signaticollis and a hybrid with the signaticollis dominant, and the same was true with respect to the wndecimlineata. All of the beetles entered into hibernation during the latter part of August and early in September, 1904. The food plants survived the long, hard, dry season and came up in the spring of 1905 in abundance, and in June, 1905, individuals of all three types emerged and were found to be interbreeding freely. A census made of the individuals which emerged late in June gave the following results: Signaticollis Type Mid-Type Undecimlineala Type 1,027 1,744 478 which clearly indicate that through some cause the hiber- nating conditions of the location were favorable for signati- collis, but decidedly unfavorable for the wundecimlineata and for the intermediate hybrid type. These individuals were allowed to interbreed freely and produced a numerous progeny, in which the larvae were of four different types: white without spots, white with spots, yellow without spots, yellow with spots. The second generation emerged from the middle to the end of July, 1905, and showed a 184 Heredity and Eugenics huge preponderance of the s7graticollis type. The census of a random sample taken the last week in July gave the following count: Signaticollis Type Mid-Type Undecimlincata Type 1,244 1,192 307 These individuals were not removed from the colony; the census of the sample was made, the individuals put back, and the colony allowed to encounter the conditions and behavior which it would meet in a state of nature. Nine pairs, taken at random, of the undecimlineata type were bred out as pedigreed cultures during August and part of September, 1905, and gave uniformly an undecim- lineata progeny. Seven pairs of the szgnaticollis type, which were bred out, gave uniformly a signaticollis progeny, and out of five other pairs there appeared individuals of the mid-type and of the wndecimlineata type, showing that some of the signaticollis type were hybrid in character. Six pairs of the mid-type were also bred out as pedigreed stock, and showed themselves to be in every case hybrid. The third generation was produced in August and early September, t905. In this the larvae were of the same four classes, but showed a huge preponderance of yellow larvae (yIS). A count made late in August, when perhaps the bulk of the larvae had entered into pupation, gave the following results: Whs Whs ylS Vis 205 227 S40 321 The adults of Generation III emerged early in September; a census made about the middle of September gave the following: Signaticollis Type Mid-Type Undecimlineata Type 2,452 O27 218 Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 185 showing again a marked decrease in the wndecimlineata form, a lesser decrease in the intermediate hybrid type, and a much greater relative increase in the signaticollis type. These hibernated during the winter of 1905-6 and emerged in June, 1906. They were allowed to interbreed freely. The population was not seen at the time of emergence, but in the fourth generation it was observed in July, 1906, and the undecimlineata type and the mid-type were nearly absent. The census made at this time, when the first generation of the year was apparently at its height, gave the following results: , Signaticollis Type Mid-Type Undecimlineata Type 3,275 45 7 These then inbred and the colony was next seen in Septem- ber at about the middle of the month, when the census of the individuals in the colony was as follows in Generation V: Signaticollis Type Mid-Type Undecimlineata Type 1,823 6 ° These hibernated during the winter of 1906-7 and emerged in June, 1907, reproduced at once, and gave an abundant progeny which emerged as Generation VI between the roth and 25th of July. These when seriated gave the following results: Signaticollis Type Mid-Type Undecimlineata Type 2,255 2 ° The second generation of 1907 emerged late in August and early in September, and of this generation the undecim- lineata type was entirely absent, and the mid-type prac- tically so. These hibernated and when seen in the spring of 1908 only the signaticollis type emerged. Both genera- tions of r908 and both generations of 1909 have developed the presence of the signaticollis type only. 186 Heredity and Eugenics In 1908 individuals from this location were brought to Chicago and carried as pedigreed cultures in the labora- tory. They have shown a complete gametic purity as far as could be determined and none have been detected which were hybrid in character. In this colony, isolated in its location, through some process or other in hybridization or perhaps by selective factors, signaticollis has completely subjected and eliminated wndecimlineata. Inasmuch as L. undecimlineata, when protected from crossing, lives well at Cuernavaca, and the selective action is very low, I am of the opinion that the swzmping of wzdecimlineata is due to some process of hybridization. This opinion is fully justified by experiments conducted in cages which eliminate selective factors. Another experiment was begun in 10905, when one hundred individuals were taken from the standard colony of L. signaticollis at Cuernavaca, and, with an equal num- ber of L. undecimlineata, from El Hule, were planted upon a vigorous growth of their food plants in a clearing made in the Foot Hill Rain Forest, in the Paraiso district, not far from Ojos de Agua, in the Canton of Zongolica. They were observed to intercross freely, but there was a preponderance of wndecimlineata-like forms, with a few intermediates, and only small numbers of the szgvaticollis type in the first generation. The census made of the first hybrid generation was as follows: Signaticollis Type Mid-Type Undecimlineala Type ° 56 1,342 A third generation was produced in late November, and in that generation there were no sigiaticollis forms visible; there were only a few of the hybrid intermediate type, and Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 187 these all closely approximated the wndecimlineata form The census obtained late in November was: Signaticollis Type Mid-Type Undecimlineala Type (eo) Il 1,132 In 1906, 1907, and 1908 these cultures were allowed to shift for themselves, and the food plants were nearly swamped by the immigration into the glade of plants from the surrounding rain forest; in fact, the whole culture was allowed to engage in a most desperate struggle for its existence. As far as the beetles were concerned, this was simply a struggle for food. In 1908-9 the inroads which had been made by other plants had so reduced the number of Solanums that the food supply was inadequate. During these years, however, no trace of the signaticollis type had ever appeared. In 1908, material of the undecimlineata type was taken from this culture to Chicago, and there subjected to the tests of pedigree analysis, but without any trace of the signaticollis form appearing. In both experiments, however, at Praesidio and at Cuernavaca, the resulting materials were different in gametic make- up from the original species. Superficially, these stocks could not be told from the natural species, but when used as the basis of experiment under control conditions, it was found that there resulted a difference in the behavior of the subsequent hybrid generations, clearly indicating a change in the gametic constitution of these groups of individuals. A series of experiments, more conclusive and under better conditions, has been carried on, using three species: L. decemlineata, L. oblongata, and L. multitaeniata. Of these, in nature, L. decemlineata is limited solely to the 188 Heredity and Eugenics United States and southern Canada; L. mutltitaeniata entirely to the southern portion of the plateau of Mexico, and L. oblongata to the Balsas Valley and the Oaxaca- Guerrero Highlands. These species intercross freely under experimental conditions and represent the following con- trasting characters for consideration. The general ground color of the larvae of L. decemlineata is wine red, that of L. oblongata and L. multitaeniata chrome yellow. L. decem- lineata and L. multitaeniata have two rows of spots along the side in the larvae, while L. oblongata has one. L. oblongata, as shown in Fig. 60, is long and oval in outline; L. decemlineata, as shown in Fig. 60, is more rounded; and L. multitaeniata is robust in type. There are also color differences between the species, which need not concern us here. Three experiments will serve to illustrate the pur- pose of this paper. In 1905, twenty L. decemlineata, from a pedigreed cul- ture, from Chicago, twenty L. oblongata, from a pedigreed culture at Cuernavaca, and twenty L. multitaeniata, derived from an isolated standard locality in the valley of Mexico south of Guadalupe, were placed on an isolated island in the Balsas River. This island was fairly well covered with a growth of Solanum rostratum, or a closely related form, upon which all three species would feed. As far as could be discovered, the island was devoid of any individuals of L. oblongata, which occur very sparingly in that general region, and the neighboring banks of the river and the islands were all searched, but they afforded no trace of L. oblongata. These introduced beetles were allowed to breed and gave the first hybrid generation in August, 1905. In this generation only the adults were seen and of the adults we could recognize definitely five forms: (A) Those which on Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 189 inspection appeared to be wholly L. decemlineata; (B) those which appeared to be wholly L. oblongata; and (C) those which appeared to be wholly L. muiltitaeniata. There were individuals which were manifestly intermediate hybrids, in F Fic. 69.—Arranged to show some of the essential differences between the species: L. oblongata, L. multitaeniata, and L. decemlineata. (A) Showing the form and characteristic markings of the adult of L. oblongata. (B) Adult of L. multitaeniata, showing the more robust form and somewhat different type of general color pattern sharply distinguishing it from both of the other species. The elytral ground color is often dark ochre, sometimes even reddish. (C) The type of L. decemlineata used in these experiments, somewhat intermediate between the two other species in body form, and to a certain extent in markings. (D) Showing the side view of a full-grown larva, with its color pattern. The ground color is yellow and that of the adult somewhat variable. (E) Adult larva of L. mullti- taeniala, with the characteristic color pattern. Ground color is yellow as in L. oblongata, but darker. (F) Shows the characteristic color pattern of L. decem- lineata; the ground color of the larvae is wine red. form, punctation, and coloration, between L. decemlineata and L. oblongata (D); and between L. decemlineata and L. multitaeniata (E). Of these five forms a census was made with the following results: 190 Heredity and Eugenics All the individuals were allowed to remain in the colony, and interbred freely in August, giving early in September a second generation, of which the following census was made: A B e D E 46 IOI go 1,292 210 These hibernated during the winter of 1905-6, and were not seen again until September, 1906, in the fourth hybrid generation of the culture. At this time the dominant form was manifestly a combination between L. decemlineata, L. oblongata, and L. muidltitaeniata, with the oblongata- decemlincata attributes in excess of those of L. multitaeniata (a combination between classes D and E of F, and F,): A B C D E 7. 25 £2 2,210 The huge preponderance of this complex type, which was neither one nor the other of the three species, suggests at once, of course, that the results could not be due to any selective process, because the type was not one of the original types but a hybrid complex. The wintering conditions of 1906-7 were especially rigorous, at least as judged by the number of beetles that I found in that location in 1906-7, when the following census was made: A B (e D E fo) ° 4 22 This shows that during the winter practically only the hybrid combination was able to survive. These repro- duced and gave a progeny in July, 1907. An inspection was made early in August, when I found only the dominant type present in the fifth hybrid generation. A B c D E fe) ° fe) 1,877 The culture was not seen again until the spring of 1908, when a considerable number of the dominant form of the Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 191 sixth hybrid generation was found emerging. These were taken to Chicago and subjected to analytical experiments and were found to breed true, both in group and in pedigreed cul- tures, with this exception, that in both the pedigreed cultures there occurred from time to time sporadic variants often standing a considerable distance apart from the rest of the population, which, when inbred, either with sports like themselves, or back to the parent type, gave behaviors which in every way are comparable to the behavior observed in many of the forms which are supposed to have arisen by a mutative process. These strains were kept through the years 1908 and 1909, and gave results which strongly suggest that the interpretation of a mutative period as described by DeVries in O. Lamarckiana, may well be the variability which follows complex processes of hybridization. In 1906 operations were begun at Orizaba, and in May the same three species from the same original stocks were mated. Conditions at Orizaba are decidedly different from those in the Balsas Valley. The city is 2,000 ft. higher in altitude and the climate is very different. In the Balsas Valley during the summer the days are bright and hot, with even showers. At Orizaba, in the location chosen at the foot of the Sierra Escamela, it is never above go” even on the hottest days, and the nights are always cool, owing to the downward draught of cool air from the moun- tains which flows over the valley at night. The relative humidity is high at all times, and the precipitation during the season was 74 inches. Under these conditions the crosses which were made thrived as far as certain members were concerned: the L. multitaeniata individuals were decidedly reduced by the conditions under which they were living and the L. oblongata individuals were hampered considerably, but to a lesser 1Q2 Heredity and Eugenics degree. Crossing was observed, however, among the com- ponent species in all directions, and progeny emerged in July, showing a combination to have been formed between L. oblongata and L. decemlineata, with the L. multitaeniata type and attributes wanting. The population, when examined, showed individuals which were apparently domi- nated by L. decemlineata (A) to the exclusion (as far as visible) of all others; individuals which were very clearly intermediate between L. decemlineata and L. oblongata (B); and individuals which were more or less intermediate between L. decemlineata and L. multitaeniata (C). Of these the intermediate between L. decemlineata and L. oblongata existed in by far the greatest numbers, as shown by the following proportion: A B Cc 131 397 92 Inasmuch as this experiment was conducted in a large cage and not in the open, it was manifestly impossible to utilize all the individuals which emerged, so a reduction was made for the matings for F,, excepting that any extreme or rare types were given every advantage over the more common types. The following materials were selected at random from the different groups as parents of the second generation: A B Cc 33 36 38 32 32 32 These inbred rapidly during July and at the end of August gave a second generation which was uniformly an inter- mediate between L. decemlineata and L. oblongata. B fe) 5890 Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 193 This was especially true of the adult characters. The larval characters, however, were also variable and appeared to be less blended into a homogeneous group. The culture hibernated from early September, 1906, to June, 1907. During this period a very great mortality occurred, which was due very largely, I think, to the fact that the culture would probably have reproduced a third time in 1906 if it had been supplied with food and proper conditions. These individuals in 1907 reproduced and gave a pretty uniform progeny of the blended type between L. decemli- neata and L. oblongata, Generation III: A B é 2 476 fe) A fourth generation was obtained in tate August and early September of the same year, which possessed the same attributes as the third generation. In nature, this culture was not carried beyond that stage, but material from the cul- ture was brought to Chicago and carried through the winters of 1907 and 1908, and the summer of 1908 and part of 1909. It was subjected to various analytical experiments, all of which tended to show that the type was a relatively stable one. Individual pairs, when inbred, gave a very definite pure line culture and groups mated at random gave the same result; but, as in the colony in the Balsas River, there appeared sporadic individuals, widely separated from the parent stock, which, when inbred, behaved in every way like DeVries’ mutants. A culture of the same material was placed at the Desert Botanical Laboratory of the Carnegie Institution in the desert of southern Arizona at Tucson, near the foot of Tumamoc Hill. In this experiment two males and two 1904 Heredity and Eugenics females of L. decemlineata, from the typical stock at Chicago, two males and two females of L. oblongata, and two males and two females of L. multitaeniata were mated in the early part of June. This culture was confined in a cage 6 ft. square on the ground and 3 ft. high, covered with wire eighteen meshes to the inch, thus eliminating all selection by insectivorous enemies. S. rostratum was supplied as food in sufficient quantity. During June and July these reproduced abundantly and gave a large progeny which emerged late in July and early in August. In this first hybrid generation at Tucson there was, as in the other cultures, a blending of the materials introduced into the experiment, but in this culture L. decemlineata was the dominant member of the cross, although not completely. In the larvae six types were observed: t. Those which on inspection appeared to be L. decem- lineata. 2. Those which were L. oblongata. 3. Those which were L. multitaeniata. 4. Those which were intermediate between L. decem- lineata and L. mutltitaeniata. 5. Those which were intermediate between L. decem- lineata and L. oblongata. 6. Those intermediate between ZL. oblongata and L. multitaeniata. It was, of course, impossible to tell on inspection what the constitution of each of these types was. Five classes of adults were recognized: A) Those which were clearly either pure, or dominants of the L. oblongata type. B) Those which were clearly intermediate hybrids be- tween L. decemlineata and L. oblongata. Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 195 C) An L. decemlineata type in which L. decemlineata was in the main dominant, but which exhibited a variable range of variability. D) Intermediate hybrids between L. decemlineata and L. multitaeniata. E) Forms which were either L. multitaeniata pure, or heterozygotes, in which L. mudtitaeniata was completely dominant. Out of 1,857 adults seriated, the following census was made: A B G D E 47 20 Tj;3rT 261 103 This census shows that while LZ. decemlineata is either the dominant or prepotent member of the combination, it did not come out of the mixture entirely without contamination. This experiment was continued in a cage exactly like the first, and the following materials were taken at random from the first generation as the parents of Generation IT: A B € D E 26 26 63 38 36 29 29 62 3% 3¢ This material immediately began breeding and gave during the month of August a large progeny which emerged early in September, and immediately went into hibernation. When seriated, this material gave the following results: A B Cc D E ° 20 247 42 fo) These passed the winter of roo8-9 in the ground and emerged in June, 1909. All that emerged were allowed to reproduce in the cage and were supplied with food as fast as it was consumed. These gave a very large progeny which appeared to be uniformly of the dominant types of the first and second generations. Seriation of the material 196 Heredity and Eugenics obtained from Generation III at the end of August, 1909, gave the following results: A B CG D E fe) 5 362 8 ° I then mated at random for the parents of Generation IV, one male of B, the only one that could be found alive, three males and three females of C, two males and two females of D, and none of E, they being absent. This material bred at once and gave in the fourth generation a considerable progeny, which were all of the dominant type. Material from Generation IV, brought to Chicago in August, 1909, placed in hibernation under experimental conditions, and brought out to breed in the middle of the winter, has shown that the dominant type is a fixed type, and that it breeds true and does not split in subsequent generations. The only splitting is that which occurs in rare individuals in from 2 to 3 per cent of the progeny, which stand apart from the general population as sports. These cases are practically the reappearance of one or the other of the component characters or combinations thereof that went into the cross, and they do not represent in this experiment anything in the way of characters new to the genus or family as DeVries states to be true of his mutants, rather they are simply the characters obtained from the different parents from which this complex has been built up. The same combination of material was made in Chicago in 1908, and was run through essentially the same pro- cedure as that of the Tucson experiment, with this difference in the result, that at Chicago L. decemlineata completely dominated the culture to the total exclusion, as far as analy- sis has been able to discover, of the presence of the other parents. Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 197 These experiments in synthesis represent what might happen in a state of nature when species which can hybridize migrate from one place to another and intercross. No one realizes better than I the complexity of experiments of this kind, the difficulties involved in the analysis of the results, and the caution that should be exercised in making statements from them. It seems certain from these experl- ments, as far as they have been carried out, and they are by no means complete, that we may definitely conclude that when like materials are combined under different natural environments, differences in the products, depending upon the conditions under which the combination takes place, result. It is certain that the type which came out of the culture in the Balsas Valley was quite different from that which resulted from the cultures at Orizaba, and these are different from the dominant type which arose at Tucson. One point of very considerable interest is the behavior of these dominant types in exactly the way in which DeVries’ Oenothera Lamarckiana behaves, giving in each generation, a greater or less number of rather divergent individuals, which, when inbred, are found to be stable germinal varia- tions. Bateson in 1902 suggested that the mutations observed by DeVries in Oenothera Lamarckiana are in reality due to some sort of hybridization behavior. I am of the opinion that Bateson’s suspicion is probably justified, at least in some instances. I have no experience with plants, and especially none with O. Lamarckiana, but my experience with these synthetic experiments has suggested that the type of behavior which DeVries has discovered, and upon which he has built an all-inclusive theory of evo- lution, is in reality nothing more than the reappearance 198 Heredity and Eugenics from time to time of attributes brought into the strain by hybridization, and which reappear in every generation, or in frequent generations, by some process akin to Mendelian segregation. It seems unreasonable to advance, as has DeVries, the idea of a premutation period, with a gradual development of invisible pangenes, and then a final bursting of these pangenes into a full-fledged mutation period, followed by a gradual dying away of the mutation period which leaves a species in a condition in which it does not produce these sports. Rather, the explanation which Bateson suggested, and which I have shown to be capable of creation in these synthetic experiments, is far more plausible and more likely to be the real explanation of the type of behavior found. This raises a very large question—one that has been raised many times—as to whether natural species may not be hybridization complexes rather than pure line cultures isolated by some sort of selection, as has been presupposed since the time of Darwin. I have found that in nature, crossing, especially between these chrysomelid beetles, is by no means uncommon, and very frequently results in adult progeny in nature, some of which have been described as species. These natural cases of hybridization have been observed in the last half-dozen years along the edge of the Mexican plateau. Some other species of chrysomelids, from the same general region, especially some species of Labidomera, have a variability strongly suggestive of a similar origin. JI have found that Labidomera suturella Chevr., of which many sharply marked variations have been described, gives a variability in pedigreed cultures that is strongly suggestive of the species having arisen Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 199 through a process of hybridization. On the high volcanic plateau of Toluca there is another type rather closely allied to L. multitaeniata, which is also suggestive of having arisen, or of being in the process of arising, through hybridization. These conditions in nature are of course difficult or impossible to check and verify, because the past is absolutely unknown, and little or no indication of what it has been can be obtained from any source. The materials in muse- ums and the records by systematists are utterly useless for this purpose. Apparently the only way of attacking this problem is the one which I have adopted of placing colonies in isolated locations, or in cages, there to carry out the process of interbreeding and forming of hybrid combina- tions as they would occur in nature. In the last few years at Tucson a series of experiments has given an exact duplication of the ‘“‘mutation behavior,” and further, it is clearly called into operation by conditions external to the organism. At first a race was synthetized and was and is still constant, but when placed under opti- mum conditions of growth and development at Tucson it has given fourteen distinct types. Some of these had in previous experiments been tested out and are known to breed true, and others are still to be tested. With plants, Gates and Davis are endeavoring to pro- duce synthetically O. Lamarckiana from a hybridization of O. grandiflora and O. biennis, and while as yet O. Lamarcki- ana has not been produced, Davis has obtained a type which he considers to be very close thereto. It is perhaps not too much to expect that in the near future O. Lamarckiana will be experimentally synthetized. If it proves to be generally true that “mutation behav- ior’ is a sequence of synthetic composition, it does not in 200 Heredity and Eugenics any way detract from the value of DeVries’ observations, nor of the réle which types thus arisen may play in evolu- tion. It is true that the hypothetical portion of DeVries’ theory as regards a permutation period and so on, is in part true, in part not. There is a period of synthesis, ending in a uniform stem race that may endure for a long time, and subsequently this throws off from itself gametes unlike, in that there are new combinations of old characters, reappearance of long latent characters, and not infrequently new characters. There is, however, this essential difference between the conception of DeVries and the one that I have to offer: namely, DeVries regards species as pure in the old sense and arising by dichotomy, while I am convinced that prog- ress will show that synthetic combinations are largely responsible for the stem forms of generic groups, and that from these there have arisen related species or types in greater or less profusion. As far as experience goes, this production of new types is in the main, if not entirely, a product of the action of external forces upon the gametic constitution, although when once started in such a strain it seems not to cease for some time though the inciting cause isremoved. In Fig. 70 [have tried to show in diagrammatic fashion the essential differences of the two conceptions. B. The Experimental Production of Germinal Variations by the Direct Action of Different Forces I. FORCES EXTERNAL TO THE ORGANISM Incident solar radiation in its various manifestations, the water relations of organisms, density and composition of the medium, and the nature of the food stream are the common groups of forces that are apt to be modified in 201 L Constitution of Organisms TeEYTMINA Modification of C 2 i Be s sa 3 g = sl 2 =e £3 ° c - — 2.8 £5. ANY Ee A= as aa £= 2 wala 2 $2£ s 3 $2 3 beee a a g eee £ ef 6.2.8 s {223 HS gas = Je “eg eh : hase / |8a= Serr Mes s4a]2@1@Y2 OAOU ap 10 pjo jo sUOYeUTG S928 xs |Sulo2 2UoHs)2/3uls sisoyyuds jo Buruuisoq area) S E22 AIP O02 —— ooas sayua—saserey> 2]qeis Jo yuosed oy oyun «OW $2MNCS VisAIP Woy sNgiN ——gestpadorBuisuuq pue | § 8 2 A P 2 a sajaured jo WO) W>}s 24) Woy UOGONpold jo pouad y “ae ost Atp jovuonBrodseouL Sue UAS Uonnjose sed auios ut g23 j onessa; pousy 2eYyWUAS suo} W238 jo UIBUG 352 ’ = e ke J = poteg a x 4 )209 woneanpyreo¢ poueg voneny Poued uonemn-aig pousy uoneinpywoy = J — e n 5 saua3ued mau Rohs jeusarxe 41q a jO uolsneyxa age s1ayoerey> ~eqoid 93103 awos jo ynsaz se weld poued uoney qoid 0) anp uon mau 40 aiqistaut ouayiy yo soueseadde uappng wue8 ul saua8ued Jo queurdojaaag “Nut snotacsid suos eynur jo UoIessa> pousy uonenp-aig ul Woy Wye jo UIBUC) 1 . c ‘a zi F Mutation Behavior as Observed by DeVries in O. Lamarckiana and His Hypothesis of Mutation Periods f ~ te — z §, 5 = EE a Cc g Ee = esis of Mutation as a result of hybrid show the differences between DeVries’ theory of the Fic. 7o.—Diagram to rpoth sis of the mutating stem form. and the hy f mutations, origin o synthe 202 Heredity and Eugenics presence, character, or intensity, in relation to the organisms, and to these forces and relations are attributed a variable value in the production of germinal variations. All kinds of extreme demands are made upon external forces, from the conception of an organism as a plastic material which is pressed into shape, and given its characters by the stress of environment, to the opposite assertion that environ- ment acts, if at all, as a minor factor in eliminating the unfit. From logic and argument no truth may be expected, and the only hope of progress in the quest for truth in this problem lies in the domain of exact genetic research. In this chapter—which is a summary of recent advances and not a historical résumé of the whole subject—the earlier work is not discussed, because it has been so often summar- ized that good discussions of it are available in many publications. All of the older work, however, is seriously defective when considered from the viewpoint of present- day genetic investigations. IN PLANTS In the bacteria and yeasts the refined and accurate methods of investigation now used, and the fuller recog- nition of the genetic requirements have made possible studies which have given much valuable information. The work of Pringsheim, Winogradsky, Hansen, Barber, Beijerinck, Buchanan, and others stands as examples of what may be accomplished in the study of this problem in these simple organisms. In all of the observations thus far made upon these organisms response to incident forces or changed con- ditions is immediate, and departures, often of considerable magnitude in form and function, occur; but in most in- stances difficulty in fixing these modified characters has been encountered. Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 203 The well-organized experiments of Buchanan with Strep- tococcus lacticus are quite characteristic of the general results obtained in the investigation of this problem in unicellular plants. His conclusion that fluctuations cannot be fixed in bacteria, whether normal or induced, is in accord with most of the experiments of bacteriologists; neverthe- less, this result is opposed by many records, believed to be accurate, of sudden permanent departures in form and function, especially in the yeasts. After a comprehensive survey of these studies upon bacteria, Pringsheim concludes that while changes in yeasts and bacteria have often resulted from the unusual action of culture media, toxic solutions, temperature, etc., the modi- fications of form and activity are diverse, and are either permanent or transient. Especially difficult, however, if not impossible in these organisms, is the attempt to separate somatic and germinal effects, and much reasonable doubt exists as to its possibility. The important contribution from this work with bacteria and yeasts is the precise demon- stration that the departures are readily produced, and are a direct result of the incident external forces used, even in the simplest known organisms. Other low plants, such as algae and fungi, have often been subjected to exciting agencies, and while changes have resulted, these, like the modifications in bacteria, are usually transient and not permanent. It would seem that these simple forms ought to provide good material for the study of this problem, although it is possible that the low differentia- tion between soma and germ may introduce experimental difficulties yet to be overcome. In higher plants the observations of Zedebauer with Capsella are of interest, and introduce observations which 204 Heredity and Eugenics might be duplicated and extended by proper experimentation with other plants and lead to new and important informa- tion. A biotype of Capsella, bursa-pastoris, much lke taraxicafolium, lives on the low plains along the coasts of Asia Minor. It has broad leaves, white flowers, and grows to 30-40 cm. high. On the inland plateau at altitudes of 2,000~2,500 meters grows another form with a stem 2-5 cm. high, reddish flowers, xerophilous leaves, and an elongated root system. From the lowlands roads lead to the plateau, and the conditions of distribution are such as to suggest that man has been influential in disseminating this form from the lowlands to the highlands, where it has taken on the modi- fied form. Some force to this interpretation of the dis- tribution is given by the fact that seeds from the plains, when taken to the plateau, at once assume the somatic characters of the plateau type. On the other hand, plateau seeds, planted and grown at Vienna, while they have the xerophilous character of the leaves, retain the upland char- acters in flowers, and, to a large extent, in height, root system, and general habit. It is futile to attempt to draw from observations in nature such as this conclusions as to the past history or actual happenings, but such instances clearly indicate the sort of experimentation that might profitably be attempted and the type of results that might be expected. Much more concrete and accurate are the findings of Klebs upon Sempervivum, in which inflorescences were found to be capable of replacement by a single flower, and many other changes were induced as the result of external, mainly climatic, forces. More important than the fact of the departure is the fact that some changes persisted Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms — 205 through three or four generations in properly guarded cultures. MacDougal has attacked this problem from a strictly experimental standpoint in the effort to discover a cause, and to arrive at an understanding, of the phenomena of “mutation”? as described by DeVries in Ocnothera. The method which he has used is to inject solutions of various kinds into the ovaries immediately before fertilization. Later, Gager, by subjecting seeds to the action of radium bromide, has shown that in plants physical factors incident upon the germinal materials can and do produce germinal changes that are permanent and persist in undiminished vigor in subsequent generations. MacDougal’s experiments, wherein zinc salts, cane sugar, etc., were injected into the ovules of plants, show that permanent changes resulted. MacDougal’s method and the reasons therefor are as follows: Having carried on pedigree cultures with a large numbe- of species for several years and having encountered some which did and others which did not give rise to aberrant individuals, attention was directed to the possibility of inducing changes in the hereditary elements in such a manner that the qualities transmitted would be altered or destroyed. A theoretical consideration of the subject seemed to indicate that the changes constituting the essential operation of muta- tion ensued in a stage previous to the reduction divisions in the embryo sac, or the pollen mother cells. It was planned therefore to subject these structures to the action of chemical agents, not ordinarily encoun- tered by the elements in question, at a time before fertilization occurred. The tests were planned to include the use of a solution of high osmotic value, and mineral compounds, some of which are toxic in concentrated solutions and stimulating in the proportions used. The probability of success would be heightened with the number of ovules contained in any ovary operated upon, and therefore the common evening primrose, Ocenothera biennis, Raimannia odorata, a relative of it, and a 200 Heredity and Eugenics member of the same family, Begonia, Cleome, Abutilon, Sphaeratcea, and Jfentzelia, and others were experimented upon. Without recourse to the detail of the work it may be stated that the use of radium prepara- tions, sugar solutions (ro per cent), and solutions of calcium nitrate, of distilled water, with capsules of Ratmannia odorata, and zinc sulphate in a stronger solution used with Oenothera biennis (Fig. 714) was fol- lowed by very striking results. In the first-named plant, there appeared in the progeny obtained from a few capsules of one individual several individuals which were seen to differ notably from the type with the appearance of the cotyledons, and, as development proceeded, it was evident that a mutant had appeared following the injections and nowhere else, which thus had some direct relation to the operation. The characters of the newly arisen form were so strikingly aberrant as to need no skill in detection (Fig. 71B). The parent was villous- hairy, the mutant entirely and absolutely glabrous, the leaves of the parent have an excessive linear growth of the marginal portions of the leaf blades and hence become fluted; the excess of growth in the mutant lies along the midrib and the margins become revolute. The leaves are widely different in width, those of the mutant being much narrower. The parental type is of a marked biennial habit and near the close of the season the internodes formed are extremely short, which has the result of forming a dense rosette; the mutant forms no rosette by reason of the fact that the stem does not cease, or diminish its rate of elongation and hence presents an elongated leafy stem, which con- tinues to enlarge as if perennial. The first generation of the derivative came to bloom; the flowers of the mutant were closely guarded and as soon as seeds were obtained they were planted to obtain a second generation. A few plants were obtained, which in every particular conformed to the new type and exhibited no return to the parental type. MacDougal’s investigations, wherein were produced modifications that have remained stable through four or more generations, in Ratmannia, Cereus, Penstemon, and others, show fully that the method employed gives definite changes in germinal constitution. Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 207 Fic. 71.—Two plants of Onagra biennis, showing the effect of injections of zinc sulphate into the ovule. A, normal. B, the modified plant, which arose from seeds that had been modified by the zinc sulphate. (From MacDougal.) 208 Heredity and Eugenics In Gager’s experiments the action of radium rays and emanations upon plants are definite, and in some instances permanent modifications resulted (Hig. 72). In these experiments modifications by physical and chemical agents were produced which are not necessarily pathological, and some of them continued to breed true in subsequent genera- tions. Gager found that the action of radium rays upon pollen cells was to produce distortion of the karyokinetic figure to the extent that chromosomes were left entirely out of the spindle and were lost to that particular germ cell. What happens in any particular variant whose modifica- tions are inheritable has not been determined, but the sug- gestion is at least plausible that the radium emanations in some way produce a new, or bring about a rearrangement of the physiological complex which exists in the germ cell. Conceivably it may be due to the displacement of an indi- vidual chromosome, although this suggestion would need verification before it could be adopted. The modifications induced by the injections of salts in MacDougal’s experiments are not easy to understand. The cells of the ovule are relatively impervious, and there is a relatively small amount of dispersion from the seat of the wound. The results obtained, however, are not due to the effects of wounding, as shown by the fact that ovules wounded in the same manner do not produce modifications unless the salts are present; likewise, ovules stung by insects do not, as far as known, produce these results, and it is only in ovules into which chemical salts have been injected that modifications are effected. The conclusion seems un- avoidable that the salts injected produced the observed results by modifying in some way the constitution of the Tic. 72.—Showing effects of rays of radium upon plants. (From Gager.) The plants in this case are Onagra biennts, showing arrested development. The ovary was exposed to radiations of radium bromide (10,000) in a sealed glass tube for 53 hours. 210 Heredity and Eugenics germinal substance. MacDougal’s general conclusion, as to the manner of producing this result, is that the action of the injected chemicals is to accelerate or retard processes, especially those of a katalytic nature within the germ cells, or possibly, actual chemical changes in germinal substance may be affected. IN ANIMALS In animals the most satisfactory results have been obtained with higher types, while among the lower types, Protozoa, for example, changes by incident forces, though capable of production, behave in much the same manner as do changes induced in bacteria and yeasts. In some instances, variations persist for many successive fissions, but they usually occur in only one of the individuals of each pair, as in Jennings’ Paramoecium with a spine. In these in- stances there is little or no spread of the change in the popu- lation through reproduction and thus far no long-continued strains have been developed through these agencies. The condition of protozoans in the non-differentiation of soma and germ, as in bacteria, is a complication not easily over- come in experiment, and it may well be, as has been often suggested, that the entire organism is the “germ plasm.” Attempts to produce germinal changes by the direct action upon the germ of external agents have not been made by many workers. Many, it is true, have subjected organ- isms to changed conditions and obtained modifications, aberrations, but relatively few tests have been made to determine the inheritability of these changes. The recent experiments of Woltereck with Daphnia and Sumner with mice are good examples of a common type of investigation, Woltereck carried strains of Daphnia in cultures in which over-feeding was practiced for about Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 211 two years. At the end of this period it was found that the head form had changed and that when the modified form was put back into the original conditions the changed form was retained. Unquestionably, in Woltereck’s cultures a head form, different from the one with which the culture started, was present at the end of two years, and this did not revert on return to normal conditions; but the failure to carry adequately controlled parallel normal lines does not permit a decision as to whether the change is a real one, or due to the progressive selection of a biotype present but obscured in the original population. Nor do the experiments permit of a decision as to whether the effects observed were due to direct germinal modifications or to so- matic transmission. It is shown that a permanent change of the race resulted, and nothing more. Parallel cultures and much more careful experimentation would be necessary in the effort to answer the more important points. Similar in method and in results are the experiments and conclusions obtained by Kammerer on certain amphibians and Lacertilia. For example, Salamandra maculosa is ovaviparous in the lowlands, but its highland variety, S. atra, is viviparous, and the larvae are large when born and have long gills. It was found that lowland forms of S. maculosa kept without water at low temperatures showed reproductive habits and young much like those in S. atra in the alpine regions. In the lizards, temperature was found to be productive of color changes, giving dimorphism in the males of one species and in the females of another. These changes are alternative in crosses. In both experiments the results have not been carried far enough to test the inheritance thoroughly, and moreover, the conditions of experiment do not permit of any analysis 212 Heredity and Eugenics of the process at the bottom of the observed changes. In both, there are changes following altered conditions, and there have resulted changes in the organism which are known to occur in many instances, only in these experi- ments some effort was made to test the permanency of the variation and its behavior in subsequent crosses. In both, the change seems to be a germinal one, as is indicated by its behavior in inheritance. Whether the change is a di- rect germinal or an indirect one, due to somatic influence or transmission, the experiments cannot decide. Precisely similar are Sumner’s experiments in sub- jecting mice to high and low temperatures, where at the end differences were found which were attributed to the effect of the different conditions. Differences there were at the end, but in mammals so variable as mice carefully pedigreed strains free from biotypes should have been used, and adequate parallel controls should have been main- tained. In that controls of critical character were lack- ing and the possibility of biotypes was not eliminated from the stock used, the results obtained are easily attributed to gradual selection of biotypes or of actuation of latent characters, as well as to the effect of changed temperatures. As for the question of somatic influence or direct germinal effect, the experiments are not conducted so as to give proper evidence thereon and are capable of any interpretation. The experiments show, however, that changed conditions changed the stock, which change may have resulted from any of the methods suggested, and the change is appar- ently permanent although the series was too short to answer this question adequately. In insects, I have obtained modifications in various ways, some of which will be described in a later portion Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 213 of the chapter. Morgan, in Drosophila ampelophila Low, found that sex-limited variations of pink eye, etc., have apparently followed the treatment of cultures of this animal to the action of radium bromide. Loeb, however, using the same organism, found that in both experiment and control the variation described by Morgan appeared, and of course concluded that the tendency to produce this variation was already present in the race of flies used. Lutz had for several years used the same strain of Drosophila for experiment, and found much variation in wing venation. He also subjected the strain at times to different experimental conditions, and this may be responsible for the appearance of the variation found by Morgan following the use of radium, and which appeared in experiment and control in Loeb’s experiments. In Chrysomelid Beetles In the modification of the germinal constitution by experimental means it must be known as certainly as is possible whether there are in the germ potential capacities, i.e., latent characters, which ordinarily are not visible in the materials used, but which may be called into visibility, periodically or rarely, by unusual conditions. Unless pos- sibilities of this kind are eliminated, it becomes difficult in experiment to decide whether observed results are the product of latent conditions, or of the experiment as de nove variations. Moreover, experiments to show the effect of incident conditions upon the germinal material must, beyond any question, show that the effect is primarily upon the germ and not first upon the soma of the parent, and secondarily, by transmission, to the germ. If, in experiment, the soma of the parent and of the resulting 214 Heredity and Eugenics progeny be modified in the same manner, there are two possible explanations. The incident conditions may modify both soma and germ independently, and they would be similarly modified because both soma and germ represent one and the same group of potentialities; or the observed results can be explained by assuming that incident conditions first modify the soma, and secondarily, through transmission, the modification is incorporated into the germ cell; and thus be interpreted as upholding the neo-Lamarckian idea of the inheritance of acquired soma variations. It follows, therefore, that the germ cells upon which experiments are to be carried out must either be taken from the body of the parent and placed in indifferent media before being experi- mented upon, or they must be in organisms that can under- go no further somatic modifications. In this there could, of course, be no transmission of acquired variations, because no variations are acquired. Moreover, for our purpose any resulting change must be thoroughly tested by subse- quent breeding for many generations. When these organisms attain sexual maturity, they have attained all of the somatic modifications, save pathological growths, which it is possible for them to achieve; the onto- genetic development of variations has come to a standstill, the whole activity of the organism is directed to reproducing the species, and further development or divergence in any of its attributes or qualities is forever inhibited. Whatever changes occur, from sexual maturity onward, are pathological or senescent. It is possible, therefore, to chminate from these experi- ments the neo-Lamarckian factor, because the conditions of experiment were not applied until after the parents had Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms — 215 attained full sexual maturity and complete development of all qualities and attributes. A check upon the possible latent characters is more dificult, but in this material it is easily carried out in one of two ways: First, only pedigreed material was used in experiment. This material has always been tested by cross- ing within and without the species to discover, as far as possible, any characters which might be present in invisible conditions. Thus far, no attributes of this description have been discovered. Second, a check was kept upon this possible source of error in the following way: these beetles have the habit of maturing their eggs in definite rotation; that is, a batch of eggs is developed, fertilized, and laid; then a time interval elapses during which another batch of eggs is being developed, and this is repeated many times, thus giving isolated lots of eggs, each separated from the one before by a time interval of from two to sixty days, or even more, and any one of which may be subjected to experiment and the others used as controls. In many of these experiments color has served as a useful character for study and the results from the experimental modification of color may be presented first. Experimental modification of color.—Color modifications are of two distinct kinds: changes in the pigment itself, and in the localization thereof. The first is a chemical change produced by a rearrangement of the chemical activities existing between a chromogen and an enzyme which brings the color-forming compounds into existence, while the latter is a change in the localization. Variations in color usually are either accentuations or diminutions of existing color, and these changes are perma- 216 Heredity and Eugenics nent, are not pathological, and the individuals possessing them are by no means weaklings, which has been shown in the case of L. pallida (Fig. 73B), which was produced in considerable numbers as a variation of L. decemlineata Say, Fic. 73.—Some divergent types of beetles produced by subjecting the germ cells to external influences. A, normal decemlincata. B, the form pallida. C, lortuosa. D, defecto-punctata. in some of my earlier cultures. This modification represents definitely a decrease in the pigmentation values of the organ- ism, with slight changes in bodily proportions, punctation, habits, etc. It arises by subjecting the organism to rigor- Modification of Germinal Constitution of Organisms 217 ous conditions, especially high temperature accompanied by low relative humidity. Such variants have been found in nature, some of them are known to breed true, and others are produced in experiment. Fig. 74 shows a photograph of a demonstration case exhibiting the results from an experiment of this kind. Here the parent pair are shown producing the two first lots of eggs which developed into normal individuals which were in following generations true to type, while the third, fourth, and fifth lots of eggs were experimented upon and gave in each modified types, pallida and minuta, as well as some normals. These different types, when inbred, came true to type in subsequent generations, as shown. Another divergent type which arose in the same series of experiments is one in which the pigmentation is increased, that is, the amount of dark pigment in the color pattern gave the resulting individual a different appearance from that of the parent species. Both pallida and melanicum (Fig. 734) were true breeding germinal variations, and pallida when given a fair start showed itself, at least in certain cultures, to be capable of sustaining itself in competition with the parent species; melanicum, however, did not exhibit any such potentiality. Ii a form like L. pallida were to develop in an arid area it would be recognized, as I have suggested, as a step in the process of evolution, and it would be directly attributed, if it were found in nature, to the conditions under which it was living, and its existence would be explained either by natural selection, or some other of the current hypotheses. Other modifications which have arisen from L. decem- lineata, especially in the modification of the hypodermal lipoid pigments, are distinctly not the increase or decrease F +patitpom ~ (07) aA® “Pats TPO by ON XS (oa) a 2 Let OR AZ ® “pars tPoW | | @ Qa a 0 G9 “patjTPow fest . = (93) 111 8 ~(93) 111 3 Pats TPR Lt On x, Leon x3 Ll ON? XD eee ay a Ne? "xT | | | . 9 @ Go @.0°90 0 9.9.90 - (93) [1 3- (aa) “pay gst powun aa a). 11 3- AY ON x LET ON x3 ACU % SENS *O3LNSW1Yad¥a ——_ 2 LOT — “L¥U SON "XY ‘NOTLYZITILNAd DNIENG ANY 3wOsss SAMOA TYDISAHA AINZG1ION1 DNOYLE O1 WAO SHI SNTIOBrANS AB $31L3307 40 Hivd ATONIS ¥ 4O ANZSONd FHL NI qaondoud S1oga43 3HL MOHS OL OZONWYYY aS¥D NoILYYLSNOWsG * (FLNAW I wadxd (vd) A S~ (¥9) LV ON* x3 UE ON paraipod 90.5 a - ae Real ® *perzIPOR ‘patgrpouun = (vq) AI 3 = (va) at 3 (q) at 3 Ley ON’ xd ‘ LBV ON xT so 9 omee 0 “PST ETPOR “P2TZ!PON p> 0} s ) 3 3 a (a0) ouing (Wa) TUE 9 {aay ts een Weeaie Ly (ONT BEVON xa Bet con sg 09 990 9 sae Ig *paty 1pown| >’ Wo geal-metism in children or grond- ca ldrer Y N? N? N Fic. 90.—Pedigree of a family with deaf mutes (D) in a large proportion of the later generations. normal ancestry, all of the children may be normal (Fig. 92); whereas, if the normal person have defective germ 282 Heredity and Eugenics cells, half of his progeny by a feeble-minded woman will be defective. Epilepsy and feeble-mindedness may replace each other (as equivalents) in pedigrees. This is well illustrated by T T d J wh tnt op BOo; +o Fic. 91.—Pedigree of a family with a high proportion of feeble-minded per- sons (F). Squares, males; circles, females; d. inf., died in infancy —GopDarD. here joe a T 1 N@eCOORE *.@ fa rh eee da Fic. 92.—Pedigree of a family in aa ac feeble-minded grandmother married twice; by a normal husband she had normal children; but by an alco- holic, sex-offending (Sx), doubtless feeble-minded husband she had only feeble- minded children.—Gopparp. the figure (Fig. 93) in which a feeble-minded sex offender has by an epileptic daughter two feeble-minded children and one epileptic child. Many criminals, especially those who offend against the person, are feeble-minded, as is shown by the way they Inherttance of Physical and Mental Traits 283 occur in fraternities with feeble-mindedness, or have feeble- minded parents (Fig. 94). The test of the mental condition of relatives is one that may well be applied by judges in deciding upon the responsibility of an aggressor. It is to be hoped that the conservatism of the law upon this matter may be speedily overcome. Not only the condition of imper- fect mental development, but also that of inability to withstand stress upon the nervous system, may be inherited. From the studies of Dr. Rosanoff and his collaborators, it ap- pears that if both parents be subject to manic depressive insanity or to dementia precox, all children will be neuropathic also (Fig. 95); that if A ahO Lrygole TSEOOD “b unktown anenceph. Fic. 93.—The pedigree of a family in whose second generation incest produces an epileptic daughter, but by whose own father she has one epileptic and two feeble-minded children; A, alcoholic; C, criminalistic; E, epileptic; F, feeble- minded; Sx, licentious. one parent be affected and come from a weak strain, half C0 DOO0 dm d0Ho 000000000 opudéagonc Clon SUPO ROG on IO attempt 120 194 ted crarefahutelute towede Fic. 94.—Pedigree of a feeble-minded family in which criminalistic (C) and licentious (Sx) traits also appear. 284 Heredily and Eugenics of the children are liable to go insane; and that nervous breakdowns of these types never occur if both parents be of sound stock. Even the condition of general nervousness is an indica- tion of a nervous weakness that is, apparently, due to the absence of a determiner. Thus when a person belonging to a neurotic strain marries a normal person whose father died of apoplexy, some neurotic and feeble-minded children may appear in the offspring. Finally, a study of families with special abilities reveals a method of inheritance quite like that of nervous defect. a O) re) CHOONXMONOONOORN ~ Fic. 95.—Pedigree of a family in which the father’s parents (upper left) are both nervous (N) and have four nervous children. The mother is nervous; so were her father and four of her brothers and sisters, while one is insane. Of the three grandchildren one is insane (I), one epileptic (E), and one extremely nervous (N).— CANNON AND ROSANOFF. If both parents be color artists, or have a high grade of vocal ability or are littérateurs of high grade, then all of their children tend to be of high grade also. If one parent has high ability, while the other has low ability but has ancestry with high ability, part of the children will have high ability and part low. It seems like an extraordinary conclusion that high ability is inherited as though due to the absence of a determiner in the same way as feeble- mindedness and insanity are inherited. We are reminded of the poet: “Great wits to madness sure are near allied.” Evidence for the relationship is given by pedigrees of men Inheritance of Physical and Mental Traits 285 of genius that often show the combination of ability and insanity (Fig. 96). May it not be that just that lack of control that permits “flights of the imagination”’ is related to the flightiness characteristic of those with mental weak- ness or defect ? These studies of inheritance of mental defect inevitably raise the question how to eliminate the mentally defective. This is a matter of great importance because, on the one hand, it is now coming to be recognized that mental defect Swicide Soler PJurde Proei igner OB 00RBO OBO O,0, 0,0 0,0 sma bo | eee Oto mechan, saventive Bare over 2byrs, bu ability ability bun Sie hore mechan abihty, ability b00r builders, €r. Ot SF yrs. dasigning boats, Fic. 96.—Pedigree of a family with great inventive and artistic ability, in whose earlier generations appear insanity (I), suicidal tendency, and eccentricity. is at the bottom of most of our social problems. Extreme alcoholism is usually a consequence of a mental make-up in which self-control of the appetite for liquor is lacking. Pauperism is a consequence of mental defects that make the pauper incapable of holding his own in the world’s competition. Sex immorality in either sex is commonly due to a certain inability to appreciate consequences, to visualize the inevitableness of cause and effect, combined sometimes with a sex-hyperaesthesia and lack of self-control. 280 Heredily and Eugenics Criminality in its worst forms is similarly due to a lack of appreciation of or receptivity to moral ideas. If we seek to know what is the origin of these defects, we must admit that it is very ancient. They are probably derived from our apelike ancestors in which they were normal traits. There occurs in man a strain that has not yet acquired those traits of inhibition that characterized the more highly developed civilized persons. The evidence for this is that, as far back as we go, we still trace back the black thread of defective heredity. We have now to answer the question as to the eugenical application of the laws of inheritance of defects. First, it may be pointed out that traits due to the absence of a determiner are characterized by their usual sparseness in the pedigree, especially when the parents are normal; by the fact that they frequently appear where cousin marriages abound, because cousins tend to carry the same defects in their germ plasm though normal themselves; by the fact that two affected parents have exclusively normal children, while two normal parents who belong to the same strain, or who both belong to strains containing the same defect, have some (about 25 per cent) defective children. But a defective married to a pure normal will have no defective offspring. The clear eugenical rule is then this: Let abnormals marry normals without trace of the defect, and let their normal offspring marry in turn into strong strains; thus the defect may never appear again. Normals from the defective strain may marry normals of normal ancestry; but must particularly avoid consanguineous marriages. The sociological conclusion is: Prevent the feeble- minded, drunkards, paupers, sex-offenders, and criminalistic from marrying their like or cousins or any person belonging Inheritance of Physical and Mental Traits 287 to a neuropathic strain. Practically it might be well to segregate such persons during the reproductive period for one generation. Then the crop of defectives will be reduced to practically nothing. I cannot close without referring to a remarkable method of inheritance of human traits, namely, the sex limited. As everyone knows, there are certain traits, such as facial hair, which are associated with one sex; and a tendency to heavy growth of beard may be transmitted by a mother’s germ cells to her son. In this case the determiner for heavy beard does not develop in the female, but only 2s in the male, under the | . . U stimulus, as it were, ® @) N of the testicular se- cretions; or perhaps El ® 5 B) @ fal ® doe El B) in the absence of an inhibiting enzyme secreted by the ovary. But in another class of cases the inheritance is most complex. Thus usually only males are color-blind, but they do not transmit their condition to their sons. On the other hand, the normal women of this strain will have color-blind sons (Fig. 97). This has been a great mystery, but thanks to the recent studies in sex chromosomes by Wilson, Morgan, and others, it is a mystery no longer. It is explained by one fact and one hypothesis. The fact is that the male has only one sex chromosome, while the female has two. The hypothesis is that a factor for distinguishing colors is lacking in the affected male and is lost out of the single sex chromosome of such a male. Now the consequence of these two prin- ciples can be seen easily. Let the striated disk (S, Fig. 98) FIG. 97. ea ie family ae re blindness (B). 288 Heredity and Eugenics stand for the single male sex chromosome, which, by hypothe- sis, lacks the color distinguishing factor. Let the white disk (IV) symbolize the absence of a chromosome in a germ cell. Let the black disks (B) represent the two female sex chromosomes with the factor of color-sight. Then the union of S+8B gives the female sex and has the determiner for color, albeit simplex. The union W+B gives the male sex and also has the determiner for color-sight. Hence neither sons nor daughters of a color- 3 ; ; ? blind man are color-blind. If the son 5 @ @» marry a normal woman, it is clear uO @: that (since no S comes into the union) the children are normal. But if the ee ae ae daughters marry, half of the males ing sex-limited characters. Will receive the single S chromosome The circles represent sex and such will be color-blind. Thus chromosomes. F the long famous knight’s move form of heredity of color-blindness is explained. Several other traits are inherited in the same way: bleeding, imperfect development of the iris, and atrophy of the optic nerve. In all these cases unaffected males may marry with impun- ity; but females of the strain who have affected brothers should not have children. The foregoing considerations bring clearly to mind the great advance that has been made in recent years in the analysis of the inheritance of traits. At last it is possible to give definite advice to those about to marry, or who do not wish to transmit their undesirable traits. Of the method of inheritance of many traits we are still in ignor- ance. In the absence of detailed knowledge, the best gen- eral advice that can be given is this; marry dissimilars. Weakness in any trait should marry strength in that trait; and strength may marry weakness. CHAPTER IX THE GEOGRAPHY OF MAN IN RELATION TO EUGENICS: RELATIONS OF BARRIERS TO HUMAN BREEDING In the period before our Civil War, while men were looking for an excuse if not a justification for slavery the subject of the unity of man’s species was much discussed. In these later days, removed from the passions of politics, as a purely academic question the inquiry has been reopened. Ideas have changed much in the intervening fifty or sixty years and now we have to define all over again what is meant by species or race. In connection with the new ideas of heredity we have gained a new conception of species and race. We now apply the terms indifferently and say a species, or race, is an intergenerating group of individuals distinguished by the possession of one or more unit-characters. As we look over mankind we note at once the groups that have always been distinguished: the Negroes with black skin and woolly hair; the oriental race with olive or yellow skin and (typi- cally) narrow eyes; the American Indian with brown-red skin and long straight hair, and the Caucasians with white skin and high cephalic index. This naive classification may have sufficed for the dawn of anthropology, but today we recognize its insufficiency. In the group of Caucasians are hundreds of distinctive characters upon each of which a race might be founded. There are the brunette skin and *Much of the present chapter is reprinted from the author’s book, Heredily in Relation to Eugenics, and it appears here through the courtesy of the publishers of that book, Messrs. Henry Holt & Co. 289 290 Heredity and Eugenics the blonde; the straight hair and the curly; the flaxen hair and the brown and the red; the blue eyes and the dark; the straight nose, the aquiline, and the pug; the broad head and the high and the narrow; the thin lips and the thick, and so through the categories. ‘The only reason why we do not have distinct species of men distinguished by such traits is because of the extensive hybridization that man is undergoing. Everywhere, brown eyes mate with blue, black hair with flaxen, curly with straight, and so on. Man’s potential races are not realized just because of the universal interfertility of the different races and because of the mobility of man’s habitat. Now is there any evidence aside from a-priori consid- erations for testing this view? Are the potentialities that we assume anywhere realized? Is the theory of man’s universal hybridization more than a figment of the imagi- nation ? First, let us admit that evidence for the unity of, say, the Caucasian race has been offered by the biometricians. They have said if the race is homogeneous it will show itself by the biometric test. Measure a trait in 10,000 individ- uals; and plot the relative frequency of the different values found. If that frequency rises in a gentle curve from its lowest value to a maximum at some middle value and then falls again smoothly to the highest value the curve is a simple curve and this has been regarded as proving a unit- population. But it does not prove it; for we now realize that even the apparently simple curve may be the result- ant of many more elementary curves whose number dimin- ishes uniformly on both sides of the center. The elementary curves are the ones that include the fluctuations of the real units. Geography of Man in Relation to Eugenics 291 Second, the real units may be isolated by the simple process of preventing the random hybridization and ensuing breeding within the type. This result has been nearly realized in small oceanic islands, and in other isolated communities. It will be interesting to look at some of these isolated places and learn what has been produced in them. At Swans’ Island, Maine, much consanguinity in mar- riage occurs; cousin marriages are the rule. A consequence has been that the defect of feeble-mindedness is unusually common and, were the process to continue for many more generations, a race with this trait, among others, would doubtless become established. At Western Martha’s Vineyard a careful genealogical study has been made by Dr. Alexander Graham Bell and much consanguineous marriage has been found. Here is, or was, being formed a deaf-mute colony; one out of every twenty-five was already a deaf mute. At Block Island, with a population of fifteen hundred, much consanguineous marriage has occurred, and a non- fecund strain has been isolated. On the Banks off Palmico Sound consanguineous marriage occurs with extraordinary frequency; and a strain characterized by suspicion, insan- ity, and mental dulness is being formed. At George Island near Eleuthera Island, one of the Bahamas, long inbreed- ing has produced a race that tends toward dwarf stature and eye defects, including cataracts. What is true of islands holds for other isolated situations. A physician at an extreme point of the peninsula of Dor- chester County, Maryland, writes that marriages there are usually consanguineous and a race of dwarfs and cripples is being formed. Mountain valleys of the Ramapo, Cats- 292 Heredity and Eugenics kill, Taconic, and Adirondack masses show many endoga- mous centers. In one place a race of criminals is being formed; in another a feeble-minded strain; in another an albino race, and so on. There is reason for thinking that the valleys of eastern Kentucky and Tennessee are centers of inbreeding and nearly pure races are being formed there. In larger settled countries the process has gone farther. From the Chin Hills, Burmah, one hears: “Rau Vau Village has been isolated for about seven generations. It contains about sixty houses and possibly two hundred inhabitants. Of these ten are idiots, many are dwarfs, and some hydro- cephalic. A number of cases of syndactylism or webbing of hands, and brachydactyly occur.” Only slightly less important than the geographical barriers are the social. A public institution brings together men and women so intimately that marriages frequently occur after leaving the institution. Thus two persons with the same trait become parents. Almshouses in which segregation of the sexes is imperfect yield numerous depauperate and imbecile offspring, and there is reason for suspecting that sanatoria and some hospitals for the “curable” insane lead to marriage of two weak persons. That institutions for the deaf lead to the marriage of similarly defective is notorious. Thus Dr. Bell, who has long warned us of the imminent danger of the formation of a deaf variety of the human race in America, says: “I desire to direct attention to the fact that, in this country deaf mutes marry deaf mutes. An examination of the records of some of our institutions for the deaf and dumb reveals the fact that such marriages are not the exception but the rule.” The barrier of language is extremely important in pro- moting consanguineous marriages, or the matings of persons } Geography of Man in Relation to Eugenics 203 with the same defect. Thus with regard to deaf mutes, Bell says: ‘‘The practice of the sign language hinders the acquisition of the English language; it makes deaf mutes associate together in adult life, and avoid the society of hearing people; it thus causes the intermarriage of deaf mutes and the propagation of their physical defect.”” The importance of this barrier is seen among recent immigrants. These tend to herd together, largely because of a desire to be with people who speak the same language. Thus immigration, instead of directly tending to promote matings of dissimilar and unrelated blood, has, at first, an exactly opposite effect. The barrier of race is of the very greatest importance in promoting marriages of kin—especially if one race be in a marked minority, as the Negroes are in New Hampshire and the whites are in the Mississippi River bottom, around Vicksburg, or in parts of the West Indies. Finally the barrier of religious sect has been erected again and again to insure the intermarriage of the faithful only. This is illustrated by the teachings of the Society of Friends and smaller sects, such as the Dunkers, Shakers, and Amish. Of the Dunkers it is written: ‘In their early history marriage out of the church was punishable by expulsion.’ It is still frowned upon but the process of liberalization now in progress had modified the attitude of the church. In some congregations families intermarry generation after generation. But the degree of kinship is not so close that any evil results appear in the offspring.”’ Nevertheless one sees the danger that any small sect with such tenets runs. A critical study of the Amish of Pennsylvania with much marriage of kin shows a sufficient progeny of epilepsy and crippled children to serve as a tChronicon Ephratense, 96, 246. 204 Heredity and Eugenics warning that a defect is in the blood of some of the strains that in time will affect the entire sect who remain in that part of the country. MIGRATIONS AND THEIR EUGENIC SIGNIFICANCE The human species has come to occupy the entire habitable globe. This fact is mute testimony of man’s migratory capacity and tendencies. Just as the Norwegian lemming has been observed, in consequence of several years of favorable conditions for breeding in its mountain home, to spread over the surrounding territory in great bands, seeking less crowded breeding grounds; even as the army worm and the grasshopper swarm from their native territory; so man, also, under the pressure of crowded conditions, poverty, and oppression, or lured by brighter prospects elsewhere, may move in hordes to other lands that seem to offer better opportunities. Thus Asia seems to have debouched her surplus population upon Europe in the shape of the Huns during the fourth and fifth cen- turies of our era and the Turks during the fourteenth and fifteenth centuries. So the Anglo-Saxons and the Normans successively swarmed upon England. So, among savages, the Masai of Africa moved upon the neighboring tribes and established themselves over much of southeastern Africa. So in the last three centuries the Americas and Australia have witnessed the greatest migrations that the world has ever seen, hundreds of thousands annually coming from overcrowded Europe, and Asia to the ‘“‘ New World.” For us in America the phenomena of migration should have a special interest. Excepting for the few scores of thousands of Indians there was a continent devoid of a population—a clean slate upon which history was to be Geography of Man in Relation to Eugenics 205 written and where the effect of “blood” in determining that history might be traced. Since the first few scores of thousands of immigrants had the greatest influence on the ideals of the colonies they established and since their blood has had the longer time to show its effect, and since their traits have had the great- est chance to disseminate widely, they deserve special consideration. On the James River the first settlers consisted chiefly of ‘“‘discredited idlers and would-be adventurers,’ more than half of them “gentlemen” of good family but untrained in labor, trusting for a change of fortune in the new land. Even later, men, women, and children were sent by the London Company to colonize the new land and that com- pany was not particular as to quality. Even felons, mur- derers, and women of the streets were at times sent over from London to relieve the city of them, and the governor, who was a pure euthenist and seemed to think the better environment would cure their evil ways, welcomed all. But a better blood soon crowded into Virginia to redeem the colony. Upon the execution of Charles I (1649) a host of royalist refugees sought an asylum here, and the immi- gration of this class continued even after the Restoration. By this means the province was enriched by a germ plasm which easily developed such traits as good manners, high culture, and the ability to lead in all social affairs—traits combined in remarkable degree in the “first families of Virginia.” From this complex and the similar complex of Maryland has come much of the bad blood that found the retreats of the mountain valleys toward Kentucky and Tennessee to its liking and that spread later into Indiana and Illinois and gave rise, in all probability, to the Ishmael- 296 Heredity and Eugenics ites, a family of which hundreds have been supported in the almshouses and jails of Indiana. From this complex came also some of America’s greatest statesmen and war- riors, the Randolphs, the Marshalls, the Madisons, the Curtises, the Lees, the Fitzhughs, the Washingtons, and many others born with the instinct to command. Such are the descendants of the high-spirited cavaliers. It might have been predicted that the future state would be “the Mother of Presidents,” and that in a civil war the severest battles should be fought on her soil. Farther north, at Manhattan Island, a settlement was being made by another sort of people: a band of Dutch traders. The fur trade with the Indians waxed profitable. The more venturesome established trading-posts up the North River, even as far as the present site of Albany; others went east as far as the Connecticut River. They maintained friendly relations with the Indians, as the main source of their wealth, and under their protection estab- lished trading-posts, even along the valley of the Mohawk. Little wonder that such blood, under the favorable environ- ment of an admirable location, has created the commercial center of the western world. On the bleak coasts of New England were being founded settlements of idealists, men who were willing to undergo exile for conscience’ sake. They included many scholars like the pastor Robinson; Brewster who, while self-exiled at Leyden, instructed students at the University; John Winthrop, “of gentle breeding and education”; John Davenport, of New Haven, whom the Indians named “heap study-man.” Little wonder that the germ plasm of these colonies of men of deep convictions and scholar- ship should show its traits in the great network of its Geography of Man in Relation to Eugenics 207 descendants and establish New England’s reputation for conscientiousness and love of learning and culture. As it was almost the first business of the founders of the colonies of Massachusetts Bay and New Haven to found a college, so their descendants—the families of Edwards, Whitney, Dwight, Eliot, Lowell, Woolsey, and the rest—have not only led in literature, philosophy, and science, but have carried the lamps of learning across the continent, light- ing educational beacons from Boston to San Francisco. Nor is it an accident that on the soil tilled by these dis- senters from the Established Church of England should be spilled the first blood of the American Revolution. Later, to the shores of the Delaware, Penn led his band of followers, consisting of men and women whose natures were attracted to his principles of thrift, absence of show, and non-resistance. The germ plasm of his followers soon peopled Penn’s woods, and it is not due solely to chance that Pennsylvania has the largest number of homes owned by their occupants and free from debt of any state. Thus the characteristics of each commonwealth were early determined by the traits of the persons who were attracted toward them. These traits still persist in their dwindling descendants who strive to secure the preservation in the state of the ideals inculcated by their forefathers. One common characteristic these early immigrants had, which led them to leave family and friends, to undergo the trials of the long sea voyage in small ships and to settle in a rigorous climate among unreliable savages, and that was a willingness to break with tradition, to exchange the old for the new and better. This trait, that amounts in extreme cases to a wanderlust, is illustrated by the history of many a pioneer. For example, Simeon Hoyt landed in Salem, 298 Heredity and Eugenics Mass., in 1628; went in the first company of settlers to Charleston (1629); went to Dorchester (1630) with the first company of settlers there; joined the church at Scituate (1635), and built a house there; then, probably in the spring of 1636, migrated to Windsor, Connecticut Colony, which he helped found. In 1649 he was granted land at Fairfield, and in 1657 he died at Stamford. Thus in the space of thirty years Simeon Hoyt lived in seven villages in America and was a founder of at least three of them— a truly restless spirit, like many another settler, and the parent of a restless progeny! Still another example is that of Hans Jorst Heydt of Strasburg. He fled to Holland when his native town was seized by Louis XIV, married there Anna Maria DuBois, a French Huguenot refugee from Wicres, and came with her to America and settled at New Paltz on the Hudson about 1710. Schismatic dissensions having broken out in the new colony, Heydt, with others, left and settled, about 1717, in Philadelphia County, not far from Germantown, where he acquired several hundred acres of land, estab- lished a colony, built mills, and entered upon various com- mercial enterprises of magnitude. In 1731, having acquired a grant of forty thousand acres of land in the Shenandoah Valley, he migrated thither, became known as Baron Hite, and died there in 1760. One of his friends, Van Metre, who originally settled at New Paltz, had moved first to Somerset County, New Jersey, then to Salem County in the same colony; later to Prince George’s County, Mary- land, and, finally, to Orange County, Virginia. These are examples, merely, of the restlessness—often enterprising restlessness—of the early settlers, and it persists in their descendants. Geography of Man in Relation to Eugenics 299 Now all of these migrations have a profound eugenic significance. The most active, ambitious, and courageous blood migrates. It migrated to America and has made her what she has become; in America another selection took place in the western migrations, and what this best blood —this créme de la créme—did in the West all the world knows. Great cities like Chicago, with its motto “T will,” arose in a generation or two to the front rank of world metropolises, and New England, the early home of the sewing machine and the cotton gin, has yielded the palm to the Central West, the home of the reaping machine and the aeroplane. And when the best and strongest migrated the weaker minds were left behind to breed in the old homestead. A recent report of the British “Committee on Physical Deterioration” contains the testimony of Dr. C. R. Browne about conditions in the west of Ireland. He says: ‘The sound and the healthy—the young men and young women from the rural districts emigrate to America in tremendous numbers, and it is only the more enterprising and the more active that go, as a rule.” And Dr. Kelly, the Roman Catholic bishop of Ross testified: “ For a considerable number of years it has been only the strong and vigorous that go— the old people and the weaklings remain behind in Ireland.” And even in New England we see signs of decadence of the old stock and men speak of racial deterioration. But the race as a whole has not deteriorated but only the New England representatives—the left-behinds of the grand old families, whose stronger members went west. Likewise in the rural and semi-rural population within a hundred miles of our great cities, we find a disproportion of the indolent, the alcoholic, the feeble-minded, the ne’er- 300 Heredity and Eugenics do-well. Thus our great cities lure to themselves the best of the rural protoplasm and surround it with conditions that discourage reproduction, either by creating a disinclination to marriage or making it inconvenient and expensive to have children. So our great cities act anti-eugenically, sterilizing the best and leaving the worst to reproduce their like. THE INFLUENCE OF THE SINGLE GERM PLASM ON THE RACE As one stands at Ellis Island and sees pass the stream of persons, sometimes five thousand in a day, who go through that portal to enter the United States and, for the most part, to become incorporated into it, one is apt to lose sight of the potential importance to this nation of the individual, or, more strictly, the germ plasm that he or she carries. Yet the study of extensive pedigrees warns us of the fact. Every one of those peasants will, if fecund, play a réle for better or worse in the future history of this nation. Formerly, when we believed that traits blend, a characteristic in the germ plasm of a single individual among thousands seemed not worth considering—it would soon be lost in the melting-pot. But now we know that unit-characters do not blend; that after a score of gener- ations the given characteristic may still appear unaffected by the repeated union with foreign germ plasm. So the individual, as the bearer of a potentially immortal germ plasm with immutable traits, becomes of the greatest interest. A few examples will illustrate this law and its practical importance. Elizabeth Tutile——From two English parents, sire at least remotely descended from royalty, was born Elizabeth Tuttle. She developed into a woman of great beauty, of tall and commanding appearance, striking carriage, ‘of Geography of Man in Relation to Eugenics 301 strong will, extreme intellectual vigor. On November 19, 1667, she married Richard Edwards of Hartford, Con- necticut, a lawyer of high repute and great erudition. Like his wife he was very tall, and as they both walked the Hartford streets their appearance invited the eyes and admiration of all.” In 1691, Mr. Edwards was divorced from his wife. After his divorce Mr. Edwards remarried and had five sons and a daughter by Mary Talcott, a medi- ocre woman, average in talent and character and ordinary in appearance. None of Mary Talcott’s progeny rose above mediocrity and the descendants gained no abiding reputation. Of Elizabeth Tuttle and Richard Edwards the only son was Timothy Edwards, who graduated from Harvard College in 1691, gaining simultaneously and highly exceptionally the two degrees of Bachelor of Arts and Master of Arts. He was pastor of the church in East Windsor, Connecticut, for fifty-nine years. Of his eleven children the only son was Jonathan Edwards, one of the world’s great intellects, pre-eminent as a divine and theologian, president of Prince- ton College. Of the descendants of Jonathan Edwards much has been written; a brief catalogue must suffice: Jonathan Edwards, Jr., president of Union College; Timothy Dwight, president of Yale; Sereno Edwards Dwight, president of Hamilton College; Theodore Dwight Woolsey, for twenty-five years president of Yale College; Jared Sparks, president of Harvard College, 1849-53; Sarah, wife of Tapping Reeve, founder of Litchfield Law School, herself no mean lawyer; Daniel Tyler, a general of the Civil War and founder of the iron industries of northern Alabama; Ann Maria, wife of Edwards Amasa Park, president of Andover Theological Seminary, herself as 302 Heredity and Eugenics astute a thinker as her clerical spouse; Timothy Dwight the second, president of Yale University from 1886 to 1898; Theodore William Dwight, founder and for thirty-three years warden of Columbia Law School; Henrietta Frances, wife of Eli Whitney, inventor of the cotton gin, who, burn- ing the midnight oil by the side of her ingenious husband, helped him to his enduring fame; Merrill Edward Gates, president of Amherst College; Catherine Maria Sedgwick, of graceful pen; Charles Sedgwick Minot, authority on biology and embryology in the Harvard Medical School; Edith Kermit Carow, wife of Theodore Roosevelt, and Winston Churchill, the author of Coniston. These consti- tute a glorious galaxy of America’s great educators, students, and moral leaders of the Republic. The remarkable qualities of Elizabeth Tuttle were in the germ plasm of her four daughters also: Abigail Stough- ton, Elizabeth Deming, Ann Richardson, and Mabel Bige- low. All of these have had distinguished descendants of which only a few can be mentioned here. Robert Treat Paine, signer of the Declaration of Independence, was descended from Abigail; the Fairbanks Brothers, manu- facturers of scales and hardware at St. Johnsbury, Ver- mont, and the Marchioness of Donegal were descended from Elizabeth Deming; from Mabel Bigelow came Morri- son Waite, chief justice of the United States, and the law author, Melville M. Bigelow; from Ann Richardson pro- ceeded Marvin Richardson Vincent, professor of Sacred Literature at Columbia University and the Marchioness of Apesteguia, of Cuba. Thus, numerous scholars, inventors, and publicists trace back their origin to the germ plasm from which (in part) Elizabeth Tuttle also was derived, but of which, it must never be forgotten, she was not the author. Geography of Man in Relation to Eugenics 303 The first families of Virginia.—This remarkable galaxy arose by the intermarriage of representatives of various English aristocratic families. The story of these early matings is briefly as follows: Richard Lee, of a Shropshire family that held much land, and many of whose members had been knighted, went, during the reign of Charles I, to the colony of Virginia as secretary and one of the king’s Privy Council. ‘‘He was a man of good stature, comely visage, enterprising genius, sound head, vigorous spirit, and generous nature.” He gained large grants of land in Virginia. His son, Richard, married in 1674, Laetitia, daughter of Henry Corbin and Alice Eltonhead. The Corbins were wealthy and extensive landowners in England for fourteen generations, and the Eltonheads were also an aristocratic family and extensive landowners of Virginia, holding high offices in the colony. Richard and Laetitia Lee had six sons and one daughter, Ann. Ann married Col. William Fitzhugh, a descendant of the English barons, military men, and parliamentarians of that name. Their eldest son married a Carter, and one of their granddaughters and one of their sons married a Randolph; their daughter, Mary, married George Washing- ton Parke Custis, and became the grandmother of Robert E. Lee. Richard Lee, Jr., had children who married into the families of Fairfax and Turberville. A brother of Richard, Thomas, was president of the council and at one time acting governor of the colony. He married Hannah Ludwell, descendant of a brother of the statesman, Lord Cottington; one of their sons, Richard Henry Lee, prepared at the Continental Congress the resolutions for independence; another, Francis Lightfoot Lee, was a member of Congress; and still another, Thomas, a judge of the General Court. 304 Heredity and Eugenics Another son of Richard and Laetitia Lee was Henry, who married Mary Bland, a descendant of Sir Thomas Bland, and a granddaughter of Theodorick Bland, speaker of the House of Burgesses and member of the Council. Their three sons were all members of the House of Bur- gesses and some were in the House of Delegates, in con- ventions and in the state senate. Such was the product of the first families of Virginia—statesmen, military men— the necessary product of their germ plasm. The Kentucky aristocracy.—Nearly two centuries ago, John Preston of Londonderry, Irish born though English bred, married the Irish girl, Elizabeth Patton, of Donegal, and to the wilderness of Virginia took his wife and built their home, Spring Hill. Of this union there were five children: Letitia, who married Colonel Robert Breckinridge; Margaret, who married Rev. John Brown; William, whose wife was Susannah Smith; Anne, who mar- ried Colonel John Smith; and Mary, who married Benjamin Howard. . . .. From them have come the most conspicuous of those who bear the name of Preston, Brown, Smith, Carrington, Venable, Payne, Wickliffe, Wooley, Breckinridge, Benton, Porter, and many other names written high in history. They were generally persons of great talent and thoroughly edu- cated; of large brain and magnificent physique. The men were brave and gallant, the women accomplished and fascinating and incomparably beautiful. There was no aristocracy in America that did not eagerly open its veins for the infusion of this Irish blood; and the families of Washington and Randolph, and Patrick Henry, and Henry Clay, and the Hamptons, Wickliffes, Marshalls, Peytons, Cabells, Crittendens, and Ingersolls felt proud of their alliances with this noble Irish family. They were governors and senators and members of Congress, and presidents of colleges and eminent divines, and brave generals from Virginia, Kentucky, Louisiana, Missouri, California, Ohio, New York, Indiana, and South Carolina. There were four governors of old Geography of Man in Relation to Eugenics 305 Virginia. They were members of the cabinets of Jefferson, and Taylor, and Buchanan, and Lincoln. They had major-generals and brigadier-generals by the dozen; members of the Senate and House of Representatives by the score; and gallant officers in the army and navy by the hundred. They furnished three of the recent Demo- cratic candidates for Vice-President of the United States. The quotation has a scientific value in comparison with the product of Elizabeth Tuttle. The New England family glows with scholars and inventors; the Virginia and Ken- tucky families with statesmen and military men. The result is not due merely to the difference in the character- istics of Elizabeth Tuttle, Richard and Laetitia Lee, John and Elizabeth Preston respectively, but to the different traits of the New England settlers as a whole, and the Vir- ginia cavalier-colonists as a body. The initial person becomes a great progenitor largely because of some fortu- nate circumstance of personal gift or excellent reputation that enables his offspring to marry into the “best blood.” The Jukes.—On the other hand, we have the striking cases of families of defectives and criminals that can be traced back to a single ancestor. The case of the ‘‘ Jukes”’ is well known. We are first introduced to a man known in literature as ‘‘ Max,” living as a backwoodsman in New York state, and a descendant of the early Dutch settlers; a good-natured, lazy sot, without doubt of defective mental- ity. He has two sons who marry two of six sisters whose ancestry is uncertain, but of such a nature as to lead to the suspicion that they are not full sisters. One of these sisters is known as “Ada Juke,” also as “Margaret, the mother of criminals.”” She was indolent and a harlot before marriage. Besides an illegitimate son she had four legiti- mate children. The first, a son, was indolent, licentious, and syphilitic; he married a cousin and had eight children, 306 Heredity and Eugenics all syphilitic from birth. Of the seven daughters, five were harlots and of the others one was an idiot and one of good reputation. Their descendants show a preponderance of harlotry in the females and much consanguineous marriage. The second son was a farm laborer, was industrious, and saved enough to buy fourteen acres of land. He married a cousin and they produced three still-born children, a harlot, an insane daughter who committed suicide; an industrious son, who, however, was licentious, and a pauper son. The first daughter of “Ada” was an indolent harlot who later married a lazy mulatto and produced nine children, harlots and paupers, who produced in turn a licentious progeny. Ada had an illegitimate son who was an industrious and honest laborer and married a cousin. Two of the three sons were licentious and criminalistic in tendency, and the third while capable, drank and received outdoor relief. All of the three daughters were harlots or prostitutes, and two married criminals. The third generation shows the eruption of criminality. Excepting the children of the third son, none of whom was criminalistic, we find among the males twelve criminals, one licentious, five paupers, one alcoholic, and one unknown; none was a normal citizen. Among the females, eight were harlots, one a pauper, one a vagrant, and two unknown; none was known to be reputable. Thus it appears that criminality hes in the illegitimate line from Ada, and not at all in the legitimate —doubtless because of a difference in germ plasm of the fathers. The progeny of the harlot, Bell Juke, is a dreary monot- ony of harlotry and licentiousness to the fifth generation. Two in the fourth generation there are, and two in the fifth, Geography of Man in Relation to Eugenics 307 against whom there is nothing and their progeny mostly moved to another neighborhood and are lost sight of. Very likely they have married into stronger strains and are founders of reputable families. The progeny of Effie Juke and the son of Max (a thief) show to the fifth generation a different aspect. Some larceny and assault there are, and not a little sexual immorality, but pauperism is the prevailing trait. Thus, in the same environment, the descendants of the illegitimate son of Ada are prevailingly criminal; the pro- geny of Bell are sexually immoral; and the offspring of Effie are paupers. The difference in the germ plasms deter- mine the difference in the prevailing trait. But however varied the forms of non-social behavior of the progeny of the mother of the Juke girls, the result was calculated to cost the state of New York over a million and a quarter of dollars in seventy-five years—up to 1877, and their proto- plasm has been multiplied and dispersed during the subse- quent thirty-four years, and is still going on. The Ishmaelites—As another example of a great family tracing back to a single man may be taken ‘The Tribe of Ishmael” of central Indiana, as worked out, under the direction of Rev. Oscar C. McCulloch, of the Charity Organization Society, Indianapolis. The progenitor of this tribe, Ben Ishmael, was in Kentucky as far back as 1790, having come from Maryland through Kentucky. One of the sons, John, married a half-breed woman, and came into Marion County, Indiana, about 1840. His three sons who figure in this history married three sisters from a pauper family named Smith. They had altogether fourteen chil- dren who survived, sixty grandchildren, and thirty great- grandchildren, living in 1888. 308 Heredity and Eugenics Since 1840, this family has had a pauper record. They have been in the Almshouse, the House of Refuge, the Woman’s Reformatory, the penitentiaries, and have received continuous aid from the town- ships. They are intermarried with the other members of this group, . and with over two hundred other families. In this family history are murders, a large number of illegitimacies, and of prostitutes. They are generally diseased. The children die young. They live by petty stealing, begging, and ash-gathering. In summer they “gypsy”? or travel in wagons, east or west. We hear of them in Illinois about Decatur and in Ohio about Columbus. In the fall they return. They have been known to live in hollow trees, on the river bottoms or in empty houses. Strangely enough, they are not intemperate to excess. Ah, that in the hordes pressing at the gate at Ellis Island, we could distinguish the John Prestons from the Ben Ishmaels of the future! THE EUGENICS MOVEMENT Since the time of Plato there have not been lacking persons who have urged that the human race would be improved were more attention paid to marriage matings. But, in recent years, these ideas have become so widespread and have been urged with such vigor, as to warrant us in speaking of a present eugenics movement. There are two chief impulses, it seems to me, for this modern movement, both world-wide. The first of these is a conviction that there is a great proportional increase in feeble-mindedness in its numerous forms—a great spread of animalistic traits— and of insanity. en a state like New York spends one- S of its state income for the care of the insane it is not strange that many of its citizens are inquiring why this is and whether there is any end to the increasing proportion of the state’s income that must be spent in caring for those who cannot aid themselves. The proportion of those who are feeble-minded in such various directions as to constitute Geography of Man in Relation to Eugenics 309 the feeble-minded class is estimated at 3 per cent of our population, and were we to include drunkards, paupers, grave sex-offenders, the criminalistic, the insane, and those with innate physical weaknesses that render them for the most part incompetent, it seems a safe estimate that 8 per cent of our population are far from having the capacities of effective men and women, able, not merely to support themselves, but really to push forward the world’s work. The cost of caring for those who cannot care for themselves because of their bad breeding is very heavy—perhaps two hundred million or more a year. A study of the cause of the increase of dependents indicates that it is because the birth rate of the better classes is constantly falling; a Har- vard class does not reproduce itself and at the present rate, one thousand graduates of today will have only fifty descend- ants two hundred years hence. On the other hand, recent immigrants and the less effective descendants of the earlier immigrants still continue to have large families; so that from one thousand Roumanians today in Boston, at the present rate of breeding, will come a hundred thousand two hundred years hence to govern the fifty descendants of Harvard’s sons! Such facts as these have awakened the people to a sense of the omnipotence of human breeding. The other impulse is the spread of knowledge of the modern principles of heredity; and an appreciation of the facts, first, that they afford a clear method in detail for improving the blood of the nation, and, secondly, that the results of this study can be set forth so simply and clearly that they may become a part of our social idealism and may serve to point the way to useful legislation. Heredity will save the people from the perdition that is to come. The realization of this fact has led to activity in various directions. In Germany, an International Society of Race 310 Heredity and Eugenics Hygiene has been organized and in England exists a Eugen- ics Education Society which publishes the Eugenics Review, and is organizing an International Congress of Eugenics for 1912. The Eugenics Education Society has fostered the for- mation of several branches in the United Kingdom. For some years Francis Galton maintained a Eugenics Labo- ratory, directed by Professor Karl Pearson, that has pub- lished a valuable Treasury of Human Inheritance, and was lately well endowed at his death. In America one of the first undertakings in Eugenics was that of Dr. Alexander Graham Bell, who was much impressed by the consequences of marriages of the deaf in America. He founded the Volta Bureau in Washington, which contains extensive records of the deaf. In 1881 Mr. Loring Moody, of Boston, who was the organizer of the Association for the Prevention of Cruelty to Children and assisted in the foundation of the Asso- ciation for the Prevention of Cruelty to Animals, organized an Institute of Heredity, but his death soon after brought his plans to naught. In October, 1910, there was started at Cold Spring Harbor on Long Island, the Eugenics Record Office, which seeks to be a clearing-house for data on human blood lines in America. It has collected several hundred records of family traits and made extensive studies into the pedigrees of the feeble-minded, epileptic, paupers, and insane. This office is publishing a Bulletin. In time we shall have there, we expect, data that will be useful to those contemplating marriage. In various directions we hope to play an impor- tant part in creating a sentiment and a knowledge that shall lead to the improvement of the blood of the American people. INDEX INDEX Albinism, 278 Alcoholism, 285 Amphibians, 211 Appetency, 10 Ascaris, 45 Bacteria, work with, 202 Bateson, W., work of, 170 Beetles, work with, 181, 213, 216 Biffen, R. H., work of, 124 Bigelow, Mabel, pedigree of, 302 Bigelow, Melville M., pedigree of, 302 Biometry, 16 Breckinridge, Robert, Col., pedigree of, 304 Brown, John, Rev., pedigree of, 304 Buchanan, R. E., work of, 203 Burbank, Luther, work of, 130 Capsella, work with, 203 Carman, E. S., work of, 130 Carow, Edith Kermit, pedigree of, 302 Castle, William E., 39, 62; work of, 112, 149 Characters, coupled and antagonistic, tor; nulliplex, 271; positive, nega- tive, duplex, 270 Chickens, work with, 146 Chromatin, 26 Chromosomes, 26, 45, 66, 287 Churchill, Winston, pedigree of, 302 Citrus fruit, work with, 128 Clay, Henry, pedigree of, 304 Correns, C. E., work of, 41 Coulter, John M., 3, 22 Cuénot, work of, 65 Custis, George Washington pedigree of, 303 Cytoplasm, 2 Parke, Daphnia, work with, 210 Darwin, Charles, work of, 39, 142, 168 Darwin, Erasmus, 9 Davenport, C. E., 269, 289; work of, 149 Davis, B. M., work of, 199 Deaf-mutism, 280 Dementia, 283 Deming, Elizabeth, pedigree of, 302 Determiner, 269; absence of, 279 DeVries, Hugo, work of, 41, 170 Dihybrids, 93 Dominance, 105 Drosophila, work with, 75, 213 Dwight, Sereno Edwards, pedigree of, 301 Dwight, Theodore William, pedigree of, 302 Dwight, Timothy, pedigree of, 301 East, Edward Murray, 83, 113 Edwards, Jonathan, pedigree of, 301 Edwards, Richard, pedigree of, 301 Edwards, Timothy, pedigree of, 301 Egg, 31, 45 Emerson, R. A., work of, 101 Environment, 9 Epilepsy, 282 Eugenics, 269, 286; geography of man in relation to, 289; organization of movement, 308; in relation to migra- tions, 294 Evolution, conception of, 4; explana- tion of, 7; fact of, 5; method of, 30; Eyes, inherited characters, 270, 276 Feeble-mindedness, 280 Fitzhugh, William Col., pedigree of, 393 314 Gager, C. S., work of, 205, 208, 222 Galton, F°., work of, 16 Gametes, 29, 62, 87, 141 Gametophyte, 34 Gates, Merrill Edward, pedigree of, 302 Gates, R. R., work of, 199 Genetics, 83 Genotype, r10 Geography, in relation to eugenics, 289 Germ plasm, 141; direct modification of, 165; influence of the single, 300; sudden transmutation in, 167 Goethe, work of, 9 Guinea-pigs, work with, 42, 149 Guthrie, C. G., work of, 146 Hair, characters of, 272 Henry, Patrick, pedigree of, 304 Heredity, 17, 42, 289; and sex, 62 Heterozygote, 62 Heydt, Hans Jorst, pedigree of, 298 Homozygote, 62 Howard, Benjamin, pedigree of, 304 Hoyt, Simeon, pedigree of, 297 Human breeding, relations of barriers to, 289 Hybridization, 118 Hybrids, 91, 122, 166 Inheritance, 42, 83, 141; sex-limited, 287 Insanity, 283 Insects, work with, 212 Ishmaelites, the, pedigree of, 307 Johannsen, W. L., work of, 85, 110 Jukes, the, pedigree of, 305 Kammerer, work of, 211 Kentucky aristocracy, pedigree of, 304 Klebs, G., work of, 204 Knight, Thomas, work of, 84 Kolreuter, work of, 84 Lamarck, work of, to Latency, 97 Lee, Francis Lightfoot, pedigree of, 303 Heredity and Eugenics Lee, Richard, pedigree of, 303 Lee, Richard Henry, pedigree of, 303 Lee, Robert E., pedigree of, 303 Leptinotarsa, work with, 181, 216 Lutz, work of, 213 MacDougal, D. T., work of, 205, 222 Macro-gamete, 64 Maize, work with, 54, 85, 116 Man, inheritance of physical mental traits of, 269 Manhattan Island and eugenics, 296 Mendel, Gregor, work of, 18, 40 Mendelism, 18, 40, 85, 100, 104 Mice, work with, 212 Micro-gamete, 64 Migrations, and their eugenic signifi- cance, 294 Minot, Charles Sedgwick, pedigree of, 302 Monohybrids, 91 Morgan, T. H., work of, 75, 222 Mutation, 13, 170 and Naegeli, Karl, work of, 40 Natural selection, 11 Neo-Darwinians, 168 Nereis, work with, 47 New England and eugenics, 296 Nitrogen, in soil, 120 Nucleus, 24 Oenothera, work with, 172, 197 Oranges, work with, 128 Orthogenesis, 14 Orton, W. A., work of, 125 Paine, Robert Treat, pedigree of, 302 Pangenesis, 142 Parthenogenesis, 67 Patton, Elizabeth, pedigree of, 304 Pauperism, 285 Pearson, Karl, work of, 16 Phosphorus, in soil, 120 Plant breeding, 113 Potassium, in soil, 120 Index Preston, John, pedigree of, 304 Price, H. L., work of, 125 Pringsheim, N., work of, 203 Protoplast, 2 Punnett, R. C., work of, 66 Reeve, Sarah Tapping, pedigree of, 301 Reichert, work of, 22 Reproduction, power of, 23; sexual, 29; by spores, 29 Richardson, Ann, pedigree of, 302 Riley, C. V., work of, 65 Russo, work of, 65 St. Hilaire, Geoffrey, work of, 9 Saltation, 171 Schenk, work of, 65 Sedgwick, Catherine Maria, pedigree of, 302 Selection, 118, 131 Sempervivum, work with, 204 Sex, and heredity, 62; immorality, 285 Shamel, A. D., work of, 124 Skeleton, inherited characters of, 277 Soil, 120 Sparks, Jared, pedigree of, 301 Sperm, 31, 45 Sporophyte, 34 Stoughton, Abigail, pedigree of, 302 Sumner, F. B., work of, 210, 212 Swimming spores, 29 Synthesis of a mutating race, 182 Talcott, Mary, pedigree of, 301 Thomsen’s disease, 278 Tobacco, work with, 114 Tower, William Lawrence, 148 Treat, Mary, work of, 65 315 Tschermak, E., work of, 41 Tuttle, Elizabeth, pedigree of, 300 Tyler, Daniel, pedigree of, 301 Unit-character, 48 Use and disuse, to Variation, germinal in animals, 210; in Chrysomelid beetles, 213; by com- bined selection and hybridization, different forces, 200; by origin of, 144; 234; by hybridization, 231; peripheral origin of, 143; in plants, 202; by selection, 238; somatic, 145 Vilmorin, P., work of, 84 Vincent, Marvin Richardson, pedigree of, 302 Virginia and eugenics, families, of 303 Von Riimker, work of, 125 295; + first Waite, Morrison, pedigree of, 302 Washington, George, pedigree of, 304 Webber, H., work of, 125, 128 Weismann, A., work of, 41, 110, 168 Whitney, Henrietta Frances, pedigree of, 302 Wilson, E. B., work of, 47 Woltereck, work of, 210 Woolsey, Theodore Dwight, pedigree of, 301 Yeasts, work with, 202 Zedebauer, E., work of, 203 Zoospore, 209 Zygote, 30, 62 ; it Ht ; i ub it Id HH At tah = =a Ss =f SS a i aE A Hibbed added di dadied, + BRAGA L A aad tnnsgdanan ia gsacteadadedsd