95^ $1.10 IN CANADA

SEEING WITH SOUND WAVES

Echoes of

Bats and Men

BY DONALD R. GRIFFIN

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

cG

IT CD

ru a

m

MARINE BIOLOGICAL UeORATORY

tito««>M««»<riM

LIBRARY

WOODS HOLE, MASS. W. H. 0. 1.

Donald R. Griffin was bom in 1915 in Southampton, New York. He was educated at Phillips Academy, Andover, Massachusetts, and at Harvard University (B.S., 1938; M.A., 1940; Ph.D., 1942), where he was variously Junior Fellow and Research Associate until 1946. Grifl&n taught physiology and zoology at Cornell University until 1953, and since then he has been Pro- fessor of Zoology at Harvard.

His enthusiasm for science began as a boy when he lived on Cape Cod. "I always found small mammals enough like ourselves," Griffin says, "to feel that I could understand what their Uves would be like, and yet dif- ferent enough to make it a sort of adventure and ex- ploration to see what they were doing. CoUege courses plus reading and conversations with an unusually wise and stimulating group of friends and advisers led my interests to include the physiological mechanisms that operate in the bodies of animals and men."

Since it soon became clear to him that many of the problems of biology might be solved by direct applica- tion of the methods and instruments of physics, he be- gan, first, to band bats, then to study and record the ultrasonic cries with which they navigate. "By a most fortunate accident," he says, "I was a student at Harvard College, where, in 1938, one of the few physicists then actively studying sounds above the range of human hear- ing was willing to let my bats register their ultrasonic sounds on his apparatus. This was G. W. Pierce, and a casual visit to his laboratory with a cage full of bats be- gan the line of research that forms the subject of this book.

"In the same years," he continues, "I was also study- ing migratory birds, first by homing experiments in which they were carried some distance from their nests and released. Many of the sea birds studied in this way

(herring gulls, terns, petrels, and gannets) found their way home. But homing experiments only tell the time required and the percentage returning at all. So I de- cided to learn to fly myself and trace the actual routes flown. I managed to do this with a number of guUs and gannets, circling in a Piper Cub for as long as ten hours at a stretch while the bird did its cross-country flying." During World War II Griflan applied the biophysical approach to projects for the development of equipment for the Armed Forces— headphones and microphones for communications, cold- weather clothing and electric suits for fliers, and studies of human vision in the infrared which were basic to the design of the infrared snooper- scope viewer.

Griffin's work, which has so advantageously combined physics and biology, has caused him to feel that his own introduction to biology and physics could have been greatly improved upon, that his early education encour- aged the misconception that "physics was the more diffi- cult and erudite of the two, and that biology was the catching, naming, and cataloguing of innumerable varie- ties of animals and plants." His later experience and re- search have forcibly demonstrated that working simul- taneously with both sciences yields original and valuable results. In fact, these studies have, Griflin says, uncov- ered "new problems faster than I or anyone else has been able to solve the old ones. I am now beginning to suspect that Hving mechanisms operate in ways that are so intricate and marvelous that if we finally understand them, we will, in the process, have extended the horizons of physics."

Dr. and Mrs. Griffin and their four children live in Belmont, Massachusetts.

61P

ECHOES OF ^r7i

BATS AND MEN

Donald R. Griffin

Published by

Anchor Books

Doubleday & Company, Inc.

Garden City, New York

1959

Available to secondary school

students and teachers through

Wesleyan University Press Incorporated

Columbus 16, Ohio

Back cover photograph by OUie Atkins. Re- printed by special permission of The Saturday Evening Post, Curtis Publishing Company, 1955. Cover design by George Giusti. Typography by Edward Gorey.

Library of Congress Catalog Card Number 59-12051

Copyright © 1959 by Educational Services Incorporated

All Rights Reserved

Printed in the United States of America

THE SCIENCE STUDY SERIES

The Science Study Series offers to students and to the general public the writing of distinguished authors on the most stirring and fundamental topics of physics, from the smallest known particles to the whole universe. Some of the books tell of the role of physics in the world of man, his technology and civilization. Others are bio- graphical in nature, teUing the fascinating stories of the great discoverers and their discoveries. All the authors have been selected both for expertness in the fields they discuss and for ability to communicate their special knowledge and their own views in an interesting way. The primary purpose of these books is to provide a sur- vey of physics within the grasp of the young student or the layman. Many of the books, it is hoped, will en- courage the reader to make his own investigations of natural phenomena.

These books are published as part of a fresh approach to the teaching and study of physics. At the Massachu- setts Institute of Technology during 1956 a group of physicists, high school teachers, journalists, apparatus designers, film producers, and other speciaUsts organized the Physical Science Study Committee, now operating as a part of Educational Services Incorporated, Water- town, Massachusetts. They pooled their knowledge and experience toward the design and creation of aids to the learning of physics. Initially their effort was supported by the National Science Foundation, which has con-

THE SCIENCE STUDY SERIES

tinued to aid the program. The Ford Foundation, the Fund for the Advancement of Education, and the Alfred P. Sloan Foundation have also given support. The Com- mittee is creating a textbook, an extensive film series, a laboratory guide, especially designed apparatus, and a teacher's source book for a new integrated secondary school physics program which is undergoing continuous evaluation with secondary school teachers.

The Series is guided by the Board of Editors, con- sisting of Paul F. Brandwein, the Conservation Founda- tion and Harcourt, Brace and Company; John H. Durs- ton. Educational Services Incorporated; Francis L. Friedman, Massachusetts Institute of Technology; Sam- uel A. Goudsmit, Brookhaven National Laboratory; Bruce F. Kingsbury, Educational Services Incorporated; Philippe LeCorbeiller, Harvard University; Gerard Piel, Scientific American; and Herbert S. Zim, Simon and Schuster, Inc.

8

PREFACE

Physical principles operate, as far as we know, through- out a universe which has both astronomical dimensions and a fine grain, some of it close at hand. New horizons can be large and distant or they may lie in the very small and commonplace. The unique properties of water mole- cules present just as interesting, even awesome, phenom- ena as does the history of stellar galaxies. And in be- tween, accessible for convenient study, is a delightful variety of ingenious mechanisms making up the living bodies of plants and animals. Man has been said to "stand between the atoms and the stars," and between molecules and men are to be found many fascinating applications of physics, broadly conceived. Outstanding among these are the ways in which living organisms utilize wave motion of various kinds. Of particular in- terest is the interplay between sound waves and the animals and men who use them.

Sound waves can teU us a great deal about the world around us, and they are often used for this purpose by both animals and men. Sound exhibits all the properties of wave motion, and these properties can be observed whenever sound travels back and forth from place to place carrying information about the things it touches. This is obviously true when people talk to one another or when birds call from the treetops. But sound waves are also useful as messengers when only one person or animal is present to broadcast them and listen for their

PREFACE

echoes a short time later. It is especially stimulating to examine the many effective ways in which animals make use of echoes, and to compare these with artificial de- vices which operate on the same basic principles. This comparison illustrates the important fact that some of the most difficult scientific questions have been solved by co-operation between different branches of science or technology. A century-old mystery of zoology was largely dispelled by one afternoon in the appropriate physics laboratory. And physicists faced with discourag- ing practical problems are inspired to believe that their tasks are not quite hopeless when they consider the accomplishments of even the smallest living brains.

Finally, there is the hope, still far from realization, that full and proper use of the physics and biology of echoes may serve to lessen the handicap of blindness. For what blind men attempt crudely, in fijiding their way about in a world of darkness, specialized animals ac- complish widi truly marvelous skill and eflBciency. Elec- tronic instruments also accomplish the seemingly impos- sible by detecting invisible targets at great distances. There is an important unity in the role which echoes play in the biology, psychology, and physics of orientation.

This account of fruitful co-operation among many dif- ferent kinds of scientists has drawn upon much of their published work. Some of this material is discussed at greater length in Listening in the Dark, and I am grate- ful to the editors of the Yale University Press for per- mission to use part of its subject matter. Readers in- terested in more detailed information will also find helpful references in the short bibliography on page 147. I have received many helpful suggestions from the staff of the Physical Science Study Committee. A large num- ber of companions and colleagues have participated in my own observations and experiments, and their aid and

10

PREFACE

encouragement were essential for the experiments de- scribed in Chapters 1 and 4. Finally, I am happy to acknowledge the patience and understanding of my wife and children who gave up many activities they would have much preferred to listening to my typewriter.

1 1

CONTENTS

The Science Study Series 7

Preface 9

1. VOICES OF EXPERIENCE 17

Echo Experts in the Ocean— Echo Experts in the Air

2. ECHOES AS MESSENGERS 35 The Nature of Sound Waves— Echoes We Seldom Notice— Water Waves and Surface Echoes

3. AIRBORNE ECHOES OF AUDIBLE SOUNDS 57

The Acoustics of Clicks and Echoes— The Velocity of Sound Measured by Means of Echoes

4. THE LANGUAGE OF ECHOES 83

Orientation Sounds of Bats— Echoes of In- sect Prey— Precision of Echolocation— Bread upon the Waters— Resistance to Jamming

5. SONAR AND RADAR 107 Echoes under Water— Prospecting by Echo- Echoes versus X-rays— Radar— Relative Effi- ciency of Bats and Radar

13

CONTENTS

6. SUPPOSE YOU WERE BLIND 129

The Sense of Obstacles— Guiding Echoes

Further Reading 147

Index 151

14

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

CHAPTER 1 Voices of Experience

Doing something in the dark is ahnost always difiBcult; the darker it is the more troublesome an otherwise sim- ple task becomes. Worst of all is to be bUnd. It is also a formidable task to build machines to trace the move- ments of distant objects which we cannot see— airplanes flying above the clouds or submarines hundreds of feet below the surface of the ocean. Finding your way on a dark night is obviously related to the problems of re- adjusting to a life of blindness, and instruments for searching out invisible targets must solve similar prob- lems. All these solutions are based on the sending out of some form of energy and the sensing of a part of this energy as it echoes back from the object at a distance. When we wish to learn about a difficult subject, such as the use of waves for searching out the invisible, we naturally look first for an expert who can explain its complexities for our benefit. There are experts who have extensive practical experience in the use of echoes. Some of them make their hving using echoes to locate small moving objects which they cannot see. One group are the physicists and engineers who design and operate

17

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

radar and sonar systems, complicated mechanisms which send out radio waves or sound waves to locate objects that return echoes of these probing signals. These sys- tems will be discussed later, but the present chapter will be devoted to another group of experts who can draw upon a longer history of reaUstic, operational experience —experts who use echoes not only to find their way but also to obtain their daily bread and butter. If their sys- tems should fail, they would starve to death, and this pressure of necessity has led to great refinement and re- habihty of their methods.

These experts are animals which Uve where sound replaces light as the best means of finding their way- caves where bats fly by the thousands, or dark waters where light is nearly nonexistent or is so difl^sed that clear images over any distance are impossible. The best known of these animal experts are the whales and porpoises, which often swim in dark or turbid waters, catching fish they cannot see, and the bats, which fly in near or total darkness, getting all their food by aerial interception of invisible flying insects. To have survived at all requhred of these animals and their ancestors enormous skill at echolocation, the location of objects by their echoes. By studying the sounds they use and how they modify them for particular problems of echo- location, we may learn much that can help blind peo- ple. Even aside from this reason, we will find these animals' use of echoes to be a fascinating subject in its own right.

Echo Experts in the Ocean

Only in the clearest water does fight travel far enough in straight fines so that objects can be seen at more than a few feet. Dayfight cannot penetrate nearly to the bot-

18

VOICES OF EXPERIENCE

torn of the ocean, though this does not mean, as people used to believe, that the ocean depths are totally dark. Oceanographers have recently discovered that lumines- cent animals are so numerous that a suflBciently sensitive light meter can register the flashes of Ught they give off when the meter is lowered far below the deepest pene- tration of sunlight. On the other hand, many rivers and lakes contain enough sediment so that clear vision is im- possible for more than a few inches even in daylight. Yet hosts of fish and other aquatic animals Uve active lives in these waters where vision is nearly impossible, and it is not surprising that some have turned to sound as a medium of communication and a means of orienta- tion, for sound travels farther in water than does light.

We usually think of the oceans and deep fresh-water streams and lakes (those without outboard motors) as silent, and few people even realize that fish or whales can hear. The chief reason is that our own hearing mech- anism is designed primarily for use in air and so does not function well in water. Our ears can detect an air- borne sound so faint that it approaches the noise level rising from random motion of molecules. The eardrums and the chain of little bones and elastic tissue that con- vey sound waves to our inner ear mechanisms are beau- tifully adapted to accept sound waves arriving through the air, but poorly suited to receive them from the water. When we do hear sounds under water, much of the acoustic energy flows directly from the water through our bodies, which are largely composed of water, to the sensitive portions of the iimer ear, where minute vibra- tions stimulate the auditory nerve.

Sound waves do not move easily from air to water, or vice versa. The boundary between a gas and a liquid acts as an almost impenetrable barrier, and more than 99 per cent of the sound energy is reflected back into

19

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

whichever medium conveyed it to the surface. That is, airborne sound waves are reflected back ahnost totally from the water, and underwater sound is equally well reflected back downward from the surface. Even if we dive beneath the water, we do not hear as well as fish can. This helps to explain why the noises made by certain fish and whales are so seldom noticed, though they have been known for centuries to fishermen and whalers. Even biologists have been slow to realize that fish can hear underwater sounds. Nevertheless, all fish have inner ears basically similar to our own, and while the sound waves reach these auditory sense organs by different routes (through the body itself rather than through air-filled canals), they stimulate the auditory nerve in very nearly the same way.

At frequencies up to about 1000 cycles per second (c.p.s.) the minimum amount of sound energy audible to a catfish is below the minimum energy detectable by the human ear. This includes the range of many musical instruments and the fundamental pitch of the human voice. At higher frequencies fish are less sensitive to sounds than we are, but their hearing is not inferior to that of land animals in any basic way.

An ability to hear underwater sounds is still far re- moved from the bUnd man's problem of learning how to use echoes for obtaining more information about his surroundings. With fish there is only suggestive evidence that certain species may utilize echoes. But marine mammals, the whales and porpoises, are not only more closely related to ourselves but also have almost as highly developed brains. Their cerebral hemispheres rival ours in size and complexity. Porpoises, which are no larger than a man, have extremely well developed inner ears and equally prominent auditory areas within their brains. Nor are they silent creatures. Once proper equipment

20

VOICES OF EXPERIENCE

was available for converting underwater sound to audi- ble, airborne sound, porpoises were found to be posi- tively garrulous. An individual porpoise has a large "vocabulary" of squeals, whistles, grunts, and rasping, clicking sounds. While fishermen and whalers had heard some of these sounds from time to time, it was only during the last war that underwater listening became re- fined enough and common enough to reveal the immense variety of sounds used by the marine mammals. Many of these sounds may be calls for signaling back and forth from one porpoise to another, but some are clearly used for echolocation.

In recent experiments individual porpoises have been isolated in small ponds or experimental tanks, such as those at the larger marine aquaria of Florida and Cali- fornia. When obstacles are set up in such a tank, the porpoises are able to dodge them at high speed, even when the obstacles are put into place on the darkest nights. While doing this, porpoises make sounds of vari- ous sorts, usually faint clicking sounds that were over- looked at first because they were masked by incidental noises in the ponds. Most porpoises spend their fives in the open ocean, but there are a few smaller kinds which live in the larger and often very muddy rivers, such as the Amazon in South America and the Ganges in India. These animals must often thread their way among un- derwater obstructions, such as logs and fallen trees. Even the species that five in open waters continue their activities at night. All porpoises feed on fish, which they must catch by active pursuit, much of the time in poor fight where it is impossible to see clearly more than a few centimeters. Therefore, it is not surprising that the most impressive feats of underwater echolocation have been exhibited in the capture of fish by hungry porpoises.

Captive porpoises are usuaUy fed by tossing dead fish

21

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

into their tank— they soon learn to swim directly to the splash from wherever they may be. The eager approach of the hungry porpoise could be explained as a simple localization of the "loud" splash which they had learned meant food. But two careful experimenters, William Schevill and Barbara Lawrence (Mrs. Schevill), work- ing at the Woods Hole Oceanographic Institution, Woods Hole, Massachusetts, in 1955, noticed that their captive porpoise found small, silent bits of food by echo- location. The porpoise spent much time searching the pond for food, and in doing so he emitted faint creak- ing noises which could be detected only with sensi- tive underwater listening equipment. They were not au- dible to a person listening from the bank of the pond or to a swimmer with his head under water. The creak- ing consisted of a series of clicks repeated at varying rates, sometimes so fast as to become a grating rasp or buzz. Suspecting that the animal might be listening for echoes from fish, Schevill and Lawrence sought to learn whether he could detect and recognize a small dead fish by echolocation and, if so, at what approximate dis- tance. To eliminate vision, they frequently worked on dark nights, and in any case their experimental pond, only about 20 meters (about 65.6 feet) in diameter, was stirred into a muddy soup by the constant swimming of the porpoise. Even a brightly painted piece of metal be- came invisible in bright sunlight when immersed to a depth of about 60 centimeters (about 23.6 inches).

When a man sitting in a small boat tied to the shore quietly held a dead fish a few centimeters under water, the porpoise learned to swim toward it, "creaking" aU the time, and seize the morsel. To make the test more critical as far as the distance of detection was concerned, a fish net was placed perpendicular to the bank, as shown in Fig. 1. The net extended out 2.4 meters from

22

■'•'.. o.- •'•lit'''?; ••••*.

':'t':::A

H

.O

O O

o ft-

-Si

tin ft.

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

the boat, and the porpoise had to decide at more than that distance which side of the net to choose in swim- ming up to the boat where food might be expected. He was fed irregularly, sometimes at one end of the boat, sometimes at the other, but in cruising past under water on a dark night he would almost never turn in closer to the bank than the end of the net unless a fish was being held beneath the surface. If the porpoise was not "creaking" as he swam past, he did not swim toward the boat even when a fish was offered.

For the most significant experiments Schevill and Lawrence sat at opposite ends of the anchored boat, each holding a fish at arm's length as the hungry por- poise swam past through the dark and murky water. Sometimes one and sometimes the other would gently and silently sHp a 15-centimeter fish just below the sur- face, and if the hungry porpoise was "creaking" as he passed by, he would usually swim in to pick up his fish. In about three quarters of the tests he would choose the correct side of the net, even though he had been ac- customed to pick up his food about equally often at either end of the anchored skiff.

This and other experiments show that porpoises can do more than simply detect isolated echoes of their creaking sounds from objects as small as 15- centimeter fish. More impressive still, they can discrimi- nate such echoes from all the other echoes that are re- turning from the bottom of the pond, the surface of the water, the bank, the net, the bottom of the skiff, to say nothing of the rocks and aquatic vegetation.

If a porpoise can echolocate a 15-centimeter fish, what prevents a blind man from hearing echoes from objects of similar size that He on the floor or on a table? Actually there are several factors which work to the disadvantage of the porpoise. Sound travels about four

24

VOICES OF EXPERIENCE

and one half times faster in water than in air, so that the time differences between echoes from objects at dif- ferent distances are that much smaller and presumably that much harder to detect. Furthermore, fish are very similar to water in the way they affect sound waves, and most of the energy of underwater sound that strikes a fish continues through its watery body just as though no fish were there. The same physical problem is present in the body and hearing apparatus of the porpoise; it too is nearly "transparent" to underwater sound, and it is intrinsically difficult for sound waves to interact with the various parts of its body. Indeed, it is likely that in the experiments of Schevill and Lawrence the echoes used by the porpoise came not so much from the 15- centimeter fish as from its small air-filled swim bladder, which reflected sound much as an air bubble would do. It would take us too far afield to present all the physical properties of underwater sound that are important for this extremely skillful echolocation by porpoises, but the interested reader may refer to Chapter 10 of Listening in the Dark (see Further Reading).

Two facts may help to explain the precision with which porpoises detect echoes from fish: one is the wide frequency range of their emitted sounds, and the other the range of their hearing. Tests have shown that they hear sounds as low in frequency as 150 c.p.s. and as high as 150,000 c.p.s. Yet these may not be the true limits of their hearing but only those set by the apparatus used to test it. In water where the velocity of sound is some four and one half times that in air, sound waves of 150,000 c.p.s. have a shorter wave length than the highest frequencies to which human ears have really useful sensitivity. Except by young children, it is doubt- ful whether frequencies above 15,000 c.p.s. are heard we'll enough to be useful for detecting objects by their

25

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

echoes. The tenfold increase in the highest audible fre- quencies shghtly more than offsets the increase in wave length of sound in water. Thus we should expect the porpoise to have only about a twofold advantage over a bhnd man owing to the shorter wave lengths that he within his repertoire of echo-generating sounds. Factors other than wave length must therefore explain the ex- pertness of porpoises at the art of echolocation, which blind men, as we shall see in Chapter 6, also try to cul- tivate. Perhaps they have simply learned individually to pay more attention to echoes, or perhaps in their long evolutionary history they have acquired ears and brains that are better adapted in some way we do not under- stand for sorting out echo components from the complex mixture of sounds bombarding their ears.

Echo Experts in the Air

Porpoises are large, spectacular, exotic, and it is rel- atively easy to accept the fact that they are capable of doing wonders in their watery world. At large outdoor aquaria they are trained to perform such circus tricks as leaping out of the water through burning hoops, catch- ing rubber balls or dead fish tossed to them by their trainers, and even throwing something back to a particu- lar person in the audience. No one who has ever watched these performances, or even motion pictures of them, can doubt the inteUigence, agility, or skill of porpoises. But they do Uve in the water rather than in our medium, the air. Consequently they seem somewhat more remote from the bUnd man's problems than the other major group of animals which make extensive use of echoes in their daily hves. These are the bats— tiny mysterious creatures and, let's face it, to many people repulsive ones. Furry little mammals, resembUng mice except for

26

VOICES OF EXPERIENCE

their wings, tliey prefer darkness, are quite at home in the blackest caves and generally encountered only as unwanted invaders of attic or summer cottage. At first glance nothing would seem to be more remote from any humanitarian contribution to the biophysics of orienta- tion of blind human beings.

It is the startling strangeness of bats, plus the folklore that couples them with demons and the nether regions, which makes it so hard to think of them with anything other than repugnance. But they are experts in the use of echoes, and if we wish to find out what can be learned about objects from echoes, we must be prepared to ac- cept important evidence regardless of our feelings about its source. It would be a real oversight to ignore the skills attained by bats in guiding their rapid flight by means of echoes.

Our knowledge of bat navigation really started in 1793 when the brilliant Italian scientist Lazzaro Spal- lanzani became interested in how various animals found their way about in darkness. Owls and other nocturnal creatures, he found, were relying on their large eyes, and they became helpless in complete darkness. But when he experimented with bats, he was puzzled to discover that they continued to fly almost perfectly when they could not possibly see a thing. Not content with experi- ments in which they flitted unconcernedly through the darkest chambers he could find, he finally resorted to blinding several bats. Even then they flew as well as ever. He released a number of blinded bats out of doors and looked for them four days later in the beU tower of the cathedral at Pavia, where he had caught them for his experiments in the first place. Wishing to know whether they had been able to continue their ordinary activities without their eyes, he climbed up to the bell tower early in the morning just after the bats' usual time for retum-

27

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

ing from a night of active flight and food gathering. Like all bats that are found in temperate cUmates, these fed exclusively on insects, flying insects which they had to pursue and catch on the wing. Spallanzani caught four of the bats he had blinded a few days earher, and on dissecting them found that their stomachs were just as tightly crammed with the remains of insects as the other bats, which had not been bUnded.

Spallanzani performed a number of other experi- ments, which, together with those of the Swiss biologist Charles Jurine, led him to conclude before his death in 1799 that, while bats could dispense with their eyes, any serious impairment of their hearing was disastrous. When their ears were plugged, they coUided blindly and at randdta with whatever obstacles were set in their way. Only a really tight closure of the ear canals sufi&ced to produce total disorientation, but Spallanzani's experi- ments were completely convincing. One example of the ingenuity of his methods was the way he investigated the possibility that the bats' navigation might be disturbed by irritation or injury caused by the earplugs rather than by interference with hearing. He had some tiny brass tubes constructed and fitted them into the ear openings of the bats. This was no easy job in the 1790s since bats' ear canals are less than one millimeter in diameter. When the tubes were in place but open, the bats could still fly with almost normal skill. When the same tubes were tightly plugged, they caused no greater irritation, yet the bats were now wholly disoriented and bumped at ran- dom into every obstacle. No matter which of several methods he used to close the ear openings, if the closure was a tight one, the bat was helpless.

On the other hand, a wide variety of other experi- ments disclosed no effect of impairment of other sense organs— vision, touch, smell, or taste. But these findings

28

VOICES OF EXPERIENCE

seemed to make no sense, for the bats were completely silent as far as anyone could tell, both before and after they had been subjected to these various experimental treatments. How could the ears replace the eyes in guid- ing their flight? In 1800 there seemed to be no answer to this question, and Spallanzani's findings were rejected, ridiculed, and almost totally forgotten. Armchair critics surmised that some refined sense of touch, probably located on the wing membranes, accounted for bats' ability to detect objects at a distance and thus avoid them, but no one even tried to explain how Spallanzani's four blinded bats had filled their stomachs with flying insects.

What came to be called "Spallanzani's bat problem" was not solved until about twenty years ago after elec- tronic apparatus had been developed at Harvard by the physicist G. W. Pierce to detect sounds lying outside the frequency range of human hearing. Just as soon as I brought some bats to Pierce's apparatus, it became ob- vious that they were emitting plenty of sound, but that it was almost entirely above the frequencies that we could hear. Further experiments, in collaboration with Robert Galambos, now of the Walter Reed Army Insti- tute of Research, showed that covering the mouth of a bat and thus preventing its emission of these high- frequency sounds was just as effective as plugging its ears. Both treatments made bats quite unable to detect large objects or small, and they bumped against the walls of the room or anything else in their path. In short, their whole orientation during flight depended upon echoes of the high-frequency sounds that they emitted almost continuously while flying about. Because these sounds have shorter wave lengths, and consequently higher frequencies, than those to which our ears respond, the ability of bats to fly in total darkness had seemed

29

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

a complete mystery. But once this simple fact became known, all seemed clear, at least in its broad outlines.

As a matter of fact, the sounds with which bats guide their flight are not totally inaudible. While more than 99.9 per cent of the sound energy emitted by bats that have been studied most thoroughly is at frequencies above the human range, there is also a faint audible component. It is so faint that one is likely to suspect the sounds come from the fluttering of the wings, and, in fact, they were overlooked by Spallanzani. Whenever a bat emits a burst of very high-frequency sound which can be detected by suitable electronic apparatus, one can also hear a faint tick accompanying each of them. Perhaps some readers may have an opportunity to watch and listen to bats on a warm evening. The bats foimd in temperate climates often roost in crevices in buildings and fly out every evening between sunset and complete darkness. If one stands close to where they fly (1 to 2 meters), and if it is really quiet and you can refrain from squealing, you can hear these ticking sounds. The younger you are, the more easily you can hear them, for even the audible component has a frequency of roughly 5000 to 10,000 cycles per second. There are also a few kinds of tropical, fruit-eating bats which make clearly audible ticks when they fly in dark caves. Where there is any light they use their eyes, which are much larger than those of the insectivorous bats. Two kinds of cave-dwelling birds also click loudly when flying in com- plete darkness but rely on vision at other times.

The faint ticking component of a bat's orientation sound is of very short duration, not imlike the ticking of a lady's wrist watch. But, unlike a watch, the bat's ticks will vary markedly in their tempo. If it is flying straight at some obstacles from a distance, there may be from five to twenty ticks per second. But if the bat is

30

VOICES OF EXPERIENCE

faced with complicated navigational problems, such as dodging you or a stick which you hold up in front of you, you may hear the ticking suddenly increase imtil it forms a faint buzz. The same thing happens just before a bat makes a landing, but the audible ticks are such a faint sound that it requires patience and completely quiet surroundings for them to be heard. The auditory basis of obstacle detection by bats was independently recognized in 1932 by a Dutch zoologist, Sven Dijkgraaf, who made a careful study of these faint, audible clicks and noted how closely they were correlated with the echolocation of obstacles. This is an example of the need for care, patience, and appropriate conditions if one is to notice and enjoy some of the more fascinating facets of the natural world.

Bats are not always agile and clever fliers; sometimes they are sleepy and clumsy— especially when they have been disturbed in the daytime. Most American and Eu- ropean species tend to let their body temperature fall to about that of the air in which they sleep. In winter many kinds of bats hibernate in caves or other places where they find temperatures only a few degrees above freez- ing. At such temperatures they are completely torpid, and one may easily think them dead. In between deep hibernation and full activity are many degrees of activity and alertness. The bats we are most likely to find and have an opportunity to observe are usually those that are least agile and least ready to display their full reper- toire of flight maneuvers. If fully airworthy, they would be xmHkely to let us watch them for long at close range. Furthermore, they often seem to become tired, and when chased about a room or attic they may soon become clmnsy from fatigue alone. But if one takes the trouble to observe bats at their best, when fully awake and in top flying condition, as they are every night of normal

31

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

summer insect hunting, then their agility and finesse at flying through comphcated pathways are truly amazing. Flitting between the rungs of a chair is easy for an ani- mal which naturally flies between the smaller branches of pine trees on the darkest nights.

After considering these two expert practitioners of echolocation and before moving ahead, let us put our- selves back into the frame of mind in which Spallanzani must have viewed these phenomena. From curiosity about the vision of nocturnal animals he had been led to perform a long series of careful and critical experi- ments with bats. While he could hear no sound as they flew about, he had convinced himself, despite his strong initial skepticism, that the ears and not the eyes were the sense organs that informed bats about such small objects as threads strung across the rooms in which he made them fly. He could make no more sense out of this con- clusion than could his critics. But he trusted experimen- tally demonstrated facts sufficiently to be convinced of the correctness of his findings, even though he could not fit them into a satisfactory logical framework. This sit- uation may arise from time to time in any branch of science, and often it means that some important new principle is just beyond our grasp. When facts fail to fit into our theories, there is usually a need to modify the theories.

Is there any reason to suppose that scientific history has just recently come to an end? Almost certainly not, and this inevitably means that new and totally unex- pected discoveries are going to be made in the future. The example of Spallanzani and the acoustic orientation of bats can remind us of several important points. First, the most rewarding discoveries may be awaiting us in what seem at first sight the most unlikely places. Second, accepted theories explaining a phenomenon have often

32

VOICES OF EXPERIENCE

proved in the past to be mistaken. There is always room for constructive questioning of even the most well estab- lished theories. Who knows what current beliefs may be shown to be as much in need of revision as the nineteenth-century view that bats felt their way through dark caves by some sense of touch residing in their wings?

33

CHAPTER 2 Echoes as Messengers

Since bats and porpoises leam so much by listening to echoes, it is important to examine the properties of sound waves that make them such useful messengers. Before sounds or other types of wave motion can tell us anything at all, they must interact with something— the surface of the earth, the walls of a house, a human lar- ynx, or the intricate mechanism of an ear that listens. Only by its doing something to some piece of matter, directly or indirectly, can wave motion be detected in the first place. Try to imagine a kind of radiation which penetrates whatever may stand in its way, traveling on and on without being changed, distorted, or deviated in its direction of travel. How could we leam that such rays even existed? High-energy cosmic rays and the sub- atomic particles called neutrinos have only the very slightest effect on matter under ordinary conditions, and, therefore, they were most difficult to discover and still are almost impossible to measure with any precision. Radio waves from natural sources have always existed at low levels of intensity and they penetrated the bodies of our ancestors just as they do our own. But only in

35

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

very recent times have men observed the interactions of radio waves with appropriate detecting instrmnents and thus learned of their existence. Suppose that everything in the world were suddenly made perfectly transparent and, furthermore, that nothing gave off light or caused it to change direction in passing from one material to another. In such a world one might as well be blind. Even though you possessed the only sense organ or de- tecting instrument capable of receiving a type of radia- tion that penetrated everything else in the universe, such special powers would be of Uttle use. Because light and sound do interact with matter around us, they are types of radiation for which sensitive detectors are useful. This is why animals and men have come to possess such effective eyes and ears.

The Nature of Sound Waves

Wave motion can conveniently be thought about in pure and continuous form: a sound having a single fre- quency, for instance 2000 sound waves or cycles per second, or light of a pure spectral color, for instance the D line of the sodium arc having a frequency of 5.1 X 10^3 c.p.s. (51,000,000,000,000 c.p.s.). Such continu- ous waves may be described quite accurately for most purposes by a graph in which the size or amplitude of the wave motion is plotted on one axis and time on the other axis. For sound of a single frequency or a single spectral color such graphs are smoothly undulating Unes called sine waves. A sine wave is the graph you would draw if you plotted the vertical motion of the hand of a clock as a function of time. Suppose you tied a string to the end of the hour hand of a wall clock, such as the one in almost any schoolroom, and tied a light weight to the other end of the string (Fig. 2). At nine o'clock

36

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

and again at three the weight will be halfway between its highest and its lowest positions, and you might draw a horizontal reference line along the wall below the clock at this level. Now suppose that at other times in the day you measured the height of the weight above and below the reference line, calling this distance a, or the ampUtude of motion of the weight. At noon the amplitude would be + a and at six o'clock it would be - a. Every six hours a would be zero, and around noon and six the curve would slow its rate of rise or fall and reverse direction. If you used the minute hand instead of the hour hand, you would plot twelve excursions of the weight during one revolution of the hour hand.

This same graph would also represent the sine func- tion studied in geometry and trigonometry. But this convenient picture of a wave leaves out any possible interactions with material objects. Hence, we shall find it necessary to think about waves in somewhat differ- ent ways or at least add to the simplified concept of a continuous sine wave before we can use it to deal ef- fectively with the message-carrying function of sounds. These special modifications add new interest to the sub- ject of wave motion as it is considered in physics courses.

What are sound waves and how do they differ from other kinds of wave motion? When sound is traveUng through some medium such as air, the pressure of the medium is changing rhythmically, increasing and de- creasing at any particular point at a rate which we call the frequency of sound (Fig. 3). To be sure, these changes may not be regular, but even when they are too irregular to be called a single frequency it is still true that alternating pressure changes occur and that the at- mospheric pressure fluctuates above and below its aver- age value, designated as one atmosphere, which one would measure with a barometer (at sea level the at-

38

ECHOES AS MESSENGERS

mosphere exerts the same pressure as does a 76-centi- meter column of mercury). Furthermore, these re- gions of slightly higher or lower pressure travel through the air, so that the zone of higher pressure which at one moment is passing a particular point will later be found some distance away. These pressure changes are much smaller than most people realize; for ex-

Fig. 3. If you could photograph the molecules of air around a source of sound, you would find some mole- cules grouped closely and others loosely. There is a high pressure in the close-packed areas and a lower pressure elsewhere. A graph of this variation in pres- sure is a sine curve.

39

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

ample, a loud shout varies the air pressure by about 0.00001 to 0.0001 atmosphere, and the faintest sound that can be heard by a normal human listener has pres- sure variations of about 2 X lO'^^ (2/10,000,000,000) atmosphere.

People used to question the existence of sound in the absence of a human listener. It was debated whether there was any sound from a waterfall in the wilderness when no one was there to hear it. This sort of question ceases to be of much importance once one distinguishes between the physical phenomenon of sound waves, pres- sure changes which travel through the air, and the subjective sensation of hearing a sound. The latter, of course, requires a listener, although an animal would do as well as a human being. But unless one believes that the waterfall and the air around it have wholly dif- ferent properties when no man is present, it is beyond question that the physical sound waves are, in fact, gen- erated as long as the water is falling.

Sound waves travel in liquids and solids as weU as in gases such as air, and while most of the time we will be dealing with sound in air, we should bear in mind that sound waves (that is, moving pressure changes) also travel through the depths of the ocean or the hardest steel. There is, however, one great limitation to the travel of sound waves. They must have something to travel through and they are barred forever from empty space or from a perfect vacuum. Pressure results from the coUisions of molecules with one another and with whatever surfaces form the boundaries of a gas, liquid, or solid. Sound can travel at appreciable intensities only where appreciable pressures exist, and this means where molecules are close enough together to collide with each other reasonably often.

The next important fact about sound waves is their

40

ECHOES AS MESSENGERS

velocity of motion. Once started, they move through a given medium at a constant rate under any particular set of conditions. They usually become weaker and weaker as they progress, and eventually die out. But as long as they are detectable at all their velocity remains the same. Nor does the velocity vary with the frequency of sound. This means that when a sound contains more than one frequency (that is, the waves have more complicated shapes than simple sine curves), the different parts of the complex sound wave move together without one component lagging or gaining on the others. The actual speed of sound depends primarily upon the medium where the sound waves travel, but temperature and other factors affect it slightly. For example, in air at 20° C sound travels 344 meters per second (about 1130 feet/ sec), and in sea water at C its velocity is about 1550 meters/sec (or 4700 feet/sec). While these distances are fairly large, they are, of course, far less than the 300 X 10^ meters (or 186,000 miles) cov- ered in one second by light and radio waves. Hun- dreds or thousands of meters are less convenient to think about than shorter distances more comparable to our own dimensions. Consequently it will often be con- venient to specify the velocity of sound in terms of dis- tance traveled in 1 millisecond, or thousandth of a second; 344 meters/sec is 34.4 centimeters, or about one foot, per millisecond, a helpful figure to keep in mind when dealing with sounds of very short duration. Another important property of a sound is its wave length or wave lengths. Wave length is the distance be- tween successive zones of maximum or minimum pres- sures as the wave travels along. Since the velocity of sound is constant, the waves, which cover 344 meters in 1 second, may either be numerous and short or few in number and longer in wave length. If the waves are

41

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

short, there are more of them in a given distance and more reach a given point in any particular interval of time, which is another way of saying they have a higher frequency. Expressed as a simple equation, velocity equals frequency times wave length (v = fxX). Or since the velocity is always the same under a given set of conditions, the wave length varies inversely as the fre- quency. A sound wave having a frequency of 344 waves or cycles per second has a wave length of approximately 1 meter; 1376 c.p.s. has a wave length of 0.25 meter, and a wave length of 2 centimeters (0.02 meter) cor- responds to a frequency of 344 ^ 0.02, or 17,200 cycles per second. High frequencies are often expressed in kilocycles (thousands of cycles) per second, abbreviated kc. A sound lasting 1 second, whatever its frequency may be, extends 344 meters from start to end as it travels through the air. A cUck lasting only 1/1 00th second is 3.4 meters from front to back. And a sentence which takes 10 seconds to utter would extend 3440 meters (more than two miles) from the speaker's mouth if his voice were strong enough to carry that far. Assum- ing that the atmosphere is dense enough to carry sound waves up to an altitude of only 30,000 meters and your voice loud enough, it is amusing to estimate how long a sound would have to last in order to make a continuous series of sound waves from your mouth up to 30,000 meters. It would be 30,000/344 or about 87 seconds, or 1.5 minutes, roughly the time it would take you to read aloud half a page from a book.

The interactions of sound waves with ourselves and the objects around us are less obvious than those involv- ing light. For example, almost every solid object casts some sort of shadow if exposed to light shining from one direction. But most famihar sounds can be heard with little change if the same shadow-casting object is

42

ECHOES AS MESSENGERS

Fig. 4. Sound does not cast sharp shadows, but it does go around corners as well as being reflected back from solid objects.

placed between the sound source and the listener's ear. Sound goes around comers more easily, and conse- quently it is more difi&cult to exclude it from a house or room or from a piece of scientific apparatus (Fig. 4). Even though sounds can be reduced in loudness by walls and other barriers, we seldom think about the degree to which they are blocked, transmitted, or reflected. So un-

43

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

familiar is this topic that we have no common words analogous to transparent and opaque to express the fact that sound waves penetrate a substance easily or not at all. Nor have we any acoustical equivalents for shiny or matte to describe surfaces which reflect sound waves chiefly in one direction or about equally in all directions.

Reflected sound waves are called echoes or reverbera- tions and they have an important effect on what we hear. We have seen that certain animals, such as bats and porpoises, fimd their way by listening for echoes. Blind men also make use of sound for orientation, and their dependence on reflected sound waves will be taken up more fully at the end of the book. But before we go on, it will be helpful to specify the meaning of a few words that are useful in describing the message-carrying ability of sound waves.

Echo generally suggests a distinct, separate reflection of a sound from some surface at a considerable distance. Reverberation implies the multiple reflections of a soimd from surfaces at closer range, so that reflected sound waves tend to overlap and become mixed with the origi- nal ones. In a more general sense, however, an echo is any sound wave that has had its direction materially changed after striking an object. When reflected waves travel through the same space as later waves from the same sound source, they interact and either increase or decrease the previous level of air pressure. If the pres- sure at a given point at a given time is increased by the presence of the echo waves, we say constructive inter- ference or reinforcement has occurred; if the soimd pressure is reduced from what it would have been with- out the reflected waves, we speak of destructive inter- ference or cancellation. These terms have just the same meaning for sound waves as for Ught.

It is important to appreciate the relationship between

44

ECHOES AS MESSENGERS

the velocity of sound, on the one hand, and the distinc- tion between reverberations and echoes on the other. In air, where we do most of our Ustening, a sound lasting one second extends 344 meters through the air, and only if it is reflected from an object more than half that dis- tance away (172 meters) will a listener close to the source receive an echo which begins after the original sound has ended. Several syllables can be uttered in 1 second— "one thousand one," for instance— and with a little effort a short syllable such as de can be repeated as rapidly as five times per second. If one spoke a single short syllable lasting 0.2 second, an echo would be sep- arated from the outgoing sound even though the reflect- ing surface was only slightly more than 34 meters (172 X 0.2) away. It is not often that we hear echoes clearly separated in time from the original sounds that created them. This is partly because we seldom deal with single sounds as short as 0.2 second, or reflecting surfaces as distant as 34 meters, and also because our ears do not distinguish two sounds as separate unless there is a frac- tion of a second of quiet between them. Even when two sounds are so close together in time that they seem to be single, the combination usually sounds different from either of its two parts if they are heard alone. Two clicks that follow each other too closely to be heard as a double click sound duller than either one all by itself. Or, if closer still, the pair of chcks may simply soimd louder than one alone.

Echoes We Seldom Notice

The echoes that usually follow every word we speak add to its quality and impact even though we are not aware of the reverberations as separate and distinct. This can be illustrated by simple experiments in which some

45

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

constant sound source is carried in and out of doors. A talkative companion might be one's first choice for a test source of sound, but he will almost certainly change the loudness of the conversation on moving from a closed room to the open air. A portable radio is better, provided that the building does not contain enough metal to act as a shield for radio waves. First one might choose an ordinary frame house for the experiment and set the volume control of the radio to a level which produces comfortably loud speech or music as the set rests on the groimd. Carrying the same radio into a small room makes it soimd much louder. Not only do the sound waves reflected from the walls add to the total acoustic energy reaching our ears, but also the announcer's voice will seem to change its quaUty because the room has selective effects on different frequencies of sound.

Of course this experiment is a crude one, complicated by many pitfalls. Perhaps there were distracting noises on the street, or at one time you may have stood closer to the loudspeaker. Perhaps the announcer happened to talk louder during the time you had the set indoors. A better experiment might involve some more constant source— a whistle, typewriter, alarm clock, or other noise-making machine, a baby's rattle or the louder kind made for use on New Year's Eve. Best of all, in many ways, is to use a tape recorder which can be carried back and forth, indoors or outdoors. In this way you can use the same sample of speech or music or perhaps make up a tape recording in which the same sequence is repeated often enough so that you can listen to it repeatedly indoors and out. If you are still skeptical, and you should be, you may wonder whether anything changes in you, the Ustener, as you move back and forth. Does your hearing become less sensitive when you are in the open air? Many careful experiments have shown that

46

ECHOES AS MESSENGERS

this is not so in ordinary circumstances. Furthermore, we can make objective measurements of sound intensity in the two places with a microphone attached to a sensi- tive voltmeter. Such measurements confirm our impres- sion that the same source of a continuous sound such as speech or music does produce a higher sound level indoors.

Let us pursue the matter a Uttle further and assume that a tape recorder is available for experiments of this type— perhaps you can borrow one from a friend or your school. It will be more useful if you have a long exten- sion cord, perhaps 50 feet in length, so that the instru- ment can be operated well away from the building as well as indoors. What sorts of sounds shall we compare in the two situations in order to learn as much as we can about the effects of echoes on how sounds sound with and without echoes and reverberations? Speech and mu- sic are excellent to get a better general understanding of these effects. But no two passages will have the same assemblage of sound waves, and it will be difficult to compare the quaUty of the different notes, words, and syllables in the same recording when heard indoors and out. With the microphone of your tape recorder you can record a sustained vocal note or one from any mu- sical instrument. It is difficult to make a recording which is really continuous and does not fluctuate in loudness. But the best solution is to splice the tape into a con- tinuous closed loop long enough to pass around both reels so that the machine plays the same sample over and over again.

This experiment wiU immediately demonstrate one important effect of echoes in a room. If a shrill, high-frequency note is maintained at a constant level in an ordinary room and if one Ustens to it carefully while moving slowly across the room, its loudness will

47

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

rise and fall at regular intervals of distance. It is of par- ticular interest to observe this effect while moving slowly near the middle of a room with the tape recorder at one end of the room playing a note that is two or three octaves above middle C. The comparison may be easier if one ear is covered so that the tone is heard entirely through the other. Listening in this way, one can usually hear clearly the waxing and waning of the soimd level and you can, with care, estimate the distance from one loud spot to the next. A meter stick hanging horizontally at about eye level may make it easier to judge the dis- tance through which the ear must move in order to pass from one point of maximum loudness to the next.

Having observed these fluctuations in the level of our recorded tone within a closed room, you repeat the same experiment out of doors. Not only will the tone sound fainter but the fluctuations will largely disappear; the loudness will fall off gradually as one walks away from the loudspeaker. Such a simple experiment as this will demonstrate that reflected sound waves from the walls of the room are interacting with those coming straight from the tape recorder and that at some places there is constructive interference producing maximum sound levels, while elsewhere there is destructive interference causing zones of relative quiet. Furthermore, it will be foimd that the distance between the maxima is one half wave length, provided that tones of nearly a single fre- quency are used. C2, the second C above middle C, has a fundamental frequency of 1024 c.p.s., or a wave length of close to 30 centimeters, and it is therefore a con- venient frequency to use for such experiments. At a lower frequency, such as 100 c.p.s., the wave length will

equal or exceed the dimensions of the room (X. = j^ =

344

-T— r = 3.4 meters), and at 10 kc or higher the successive

48

ECHOES AS MESSENGERS 344

maxima and minima (X = ^^ ^^^ = 0.034 meters or 3.4

centimeters) will be too close together for easy de- tection. Notes from musical instrmnents have so many frequencies, or harmonics, each giving its own maxima and minima at its own wave length, that it will be diffi- cult to distinguish the loud and quiet spots for each frequency. Hence, the purer the note the more obvious the effect. You will find the flute more satisfactory be- cause of its purer tone than a piano or violin.

These maxima and minima are called standing waves. A loud spot is the point where sound waves reflected from the walls add to others arriving directly from the loudspeaker. If several parts of the walls all send strong reflections to the same spot, these various echoes are likely to arrive at different times and fail to reinforce each other as strongly as they would if arriving at the same time. In some rooms of irregular shape the standing waves may thus be inconspicuous, but most rooms are regular enough and have sufficiently reflective walls so that at least in the middle of the room the standing- wave pattern is noticeable. If you have an opportunity to experiment with a ripple tank in which surface waves on water are generated to illustrate the various phenom- ena of wave motion, you will fimd that the frequency of the vibrating object producing the waves has to be ad- justed rather carefully to obtain pronounced standing waves. Otherwise the water's surface may show only a shifting and confusing mess of wavelets chasing each other back and forth without apparent order. If the tank is not a simple shape, such as a rectangle, then the standing-wave patterns are either very complicated or are limited to a few areas where reflected waves do man- age to reinforce those arriving directly from their source.

Suppose we try to set up standing waves in a room by

49

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

generating not a single frequency but a sound containing many different frequencies. Speech and music answer this description, but the different frequencies change rapidly with time, so the effects are compUcated. Still, it is true that even though we do not ordinarily notice standing waves of speech or music, in some very large rooms there may be "dead spots" where interference between the direct and reflected sound makes hstening very difficult and unpleasant. Indeed, there is a whole science of architectural acoustics devoted to minimizing such "dead spots" and to controlling the echoes from the walls of auditoria so that speech and music are car- ried as faithfully as possible to all parts of the hall.

A simple experiment with our tape recorder in an ordinary room can demonstrate the effects of having many frequencies present at the same time. A loud hiss made vocally into the microphone will, when played back, fill the room with a still louder hiss. But you will probably have great difficulty in hearing standing waves. The same experiment can sometimes be performed by turning up the volume control of a radio or record player until you hear a hissing sound that incidentally stems from the random motions of molecules in some part of the electronic circuit. It has a wide range or band of frequencies, as does a vocal hiss, and all are about equally loud. So many different wave lengths are present that even though each one tends to set up standing waves at its own wave length, all the others have equally strong tendencies to estabHsh loud spots separated by their wave lengths. The result is that the over-all level of the sound is much the same from point to point within the room. To obtain clear standing waves there must be only one or a very few wave lengths prominent in the sound that fills the room.

Perhaps when you are listening for standing waves

50

ECHOES AS MESSENGERS

someone else may walk into the room. Often this will cause a shift in the positions of the maxima and minima even though the room is fairly large. This is a more compHcated type of interaction in which the exact loca- tion of greatest interference is influenced by all sorts of objects that add reflected sound waves to those arriving directly from the tape recorder. Because of these changes the standing-wave patterns could be used to tell us that someone had entered the room. Usually we have much easier ways of knowing this, but there are circumstances where changes in standing-wave patterns have been put to use to detect small changes in the position of objects in a room. One type of burglar alarm operates on this principle. Suppose you were walking about blindfolded in the same room. The standing- wave pattern would at least inform you that you were in a room and not out of doors where nothing was reflecting enough sound to set up standing waves. It could also tell you when some- thing else moved into the room, provided you were standing still and noticed the shift in the standing waves. These may seem to be trivial examples, but blind men do learn to pay attention to many aspects of the sound fields in which they live and in this way learn much about what goes on around them. Remember, too, that these examples have been selected for their simplicity, and from such crude beginnings we can go on to much more difficult questions that can be answered by carrying these experiments further. This is in essence to use sound waves as tools or "sense extenders" for exploring one's surroundings. Crude tools used with little skill yield only crude information. But, as we already have seen, even such small animals as bats have become ex- pert at using sound waves as tools of this sort to learn rather complicated facts about what goes on around them. They have come to do this in the long course of

51

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

their evolutionary history because they live and move under conditions where sound is a convenient or per- haps the only available means for maintaining their orientation.

Telling whether you are indoors or out on the street by listening to a tape recording of a shrill and monoto- nous tone may seem a clumsy way to accompUsh the obvious. But transpose the situation to a man lost in a pitch-black cave and unable to use a hght of any kind. Sound waves would be one of the most useful means, if not the only means, at his disposal to learn about those parts of the cave beyond the direct reach of his out- stretched hands and feet. Bats do not feel their way; they fly rapidly through complex and tortuous passages of a cave, dodging stalactites and other bats without acci- dents of any kind, and, as I shall explain later, this is one of the less difficult of the many tasks these little animals accomplish by means of sound waves.

In pursuing these matters further it will be best to return from time to time to the simple experiments with audible sounds such as those we have just conducted. In this way you may have firsthand experience to confirm and support the concepts and theories about which you read. For many purposes the ripple tank used in physics courses provides more convenient types of waves with which the same phenomena can equally well be visual- ized. This is basically because surface waves on water travel slowly enough for you to watch them directly. Furthermore, their velocity varies with the depth of the water, and they can be caused to bend by installing shallow "sandbars" or "reefs" in the ripple tank. The same ripple tank can also be used to study echoes which are closely analogous to those that cause standing waves of sound and to those used by bats or men to find their way about in situations where hght is not available.

52

ECHOES AS MESSENGERS

Water Waves and Surface Echoes

There are many and detailed parallels between water waves and sound waves in air or, for that matter, light waves; but aside from their serving as slow-motion models, we are likely to think of surface waves on water as of little interest and certainly as wholly devoid of the abiUty to carry information. Who would think of trying to signal back and forth across the ocean by means of water waves? They die out too soon and are too easily confused with the natural waves from winds or water currents. A leaf that falls to the surface of a quiet pond may produce a few ripples, but how could one hope to detect this event a hundred feet away? Yet from their very similarity to sound or light we might expect the water waves we study in the physics laboratory to have some message-carrying function. Such cases can be found if we look for them in nature and, in this instance, the search leads to the so-called whirligig beetle which bridges the gap between the ripple tank and the most compUcated radar installations.

Whirligig beetles are common inhabitants of small ponds and quiet streams. While these aquatic insects often dive and swim below the surface, they are usually noticed most easily when darting about on the surface film of the water. They are light enough in weight so that they are supported by the surface tension of the water— largely because of their fringe of hairs covered with a thin film of waxy material that does not readily become wetted. This ability to support themselves on water could easily lead us into a digression about sur- face tension and why water is a uniquely suitable liquid for the flotation of water beetles. But this is a subject

53

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

which is covered well in the Science Study Series book Soap Bubbles,

More pertinent is the fact that the water beetles make use of surface waves to keep themselves posted about the proximity of the water's edge. They have eyes and use them under many conditions, but at night or when vision is prevented by laboratory experiments performed in darkness they still manage quite skillfully to avoid collisions with the edge of an aquarium and with each other. A German biologist named Friedrich Eggers

Fig. 5. A whirligig beetle whose other legs lie beneath its body is able to sense water waves and their echoes with the two specialized antennae which protrude from the head and float on the water. It is also interesting to note that this beetle has four compound eyes, two above the water and two below.

Studied these beetles with great care in the 1920s. Un- like those of most insects, their antennae, or feelers are shaped in an especially suitable way to float on the sur- face film of the water. The numerous hairs all arranged

54

ECHOES AS MESSENGERS

parallel to each other are at just the correct angle so that they float in the surface film. But more specialized still are the hairs located on one particular joint of each an- tenna, the second from the base. These specialized hairs are more than flotation devices; at the base are sensitive nerves that are stimulated by the most minute move- ments of the hairs relative to the remainder of the beetle. Eggers surmised from the microscopic structure of these hairs and nerves (see Fig. 5) that they were used to detect motion of the water surface, and he therefore experimented with them directly. In some beetles he damaged the second segments of the antennae, cut off the hairs on this portion, or damaged only the nerves leading from the bases of these hairs into the central nervous system of the insect. When these water beetles were placed on the surface of an aquarium in the dark, they acted as bewildered as a bird fluttering against a windowpane and collided at random with the walls.

Other experiments have shown that the sense organs of insects can respond to very weak vibrations. A move- ment of as little as 4 X 10"® centimeter is detected by the sensory nerves attached to fine hairs on the surface of some insects which are generally similar in structure to the whirhgig water beetles. There is thus no reason to be amazed that water beetles can feel the surface waves generated by their own swimming or walking movements. What is amazing is their ability to discrimi- nate the jiggling that results from reflected waves from all the other vibrations that must be affecting the same hairs and the same sensory nerves. This is a problem which the beetles may avoid to a considerable extent by their habit of swimming intermittently, with frequent pauses during which they may perhaps be feeling the "reverberations" of the water waves their swimming has

55

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

generated a fraction of a second earlier. But the naviga- tion of water beetles has not been studied since Eggers' day, and it is typical of the opportunities that await patient and ingenious students of biophysics. In the fol- lowing chapters I shall describe in more detail better known examples of animals' and men's learning a great deal by listening for echoes, and it will become apparent that living sense organs and brains detect echoes that seem on first thought far too faint to be of any possible use. The phenomenon is basically one of discrimination, or sifting out faint but important echoes from much stronger waves of the same type which are not relevant for the purposes of the particular animal. Living nervous systems are superior to artificial machines in making a wide variety of fine discriminations, and the next chap- ter describes experiments you can perform yourself to show how the human ear and brain discriminate among various types of sounds including echoes.

56

CHAPTER 3 Airborne Echoes of Audible Sounds

The word "echo" suggests a quiet country scene where a steep cliff or hillside looms up hundreds of feet away. A shout or a gunshot suddenly breaks the silence, and there follows a repetition of the sound, fainter than the original. Knowing the velocity of sound, we could de- termine our distance from the hillside if we accurately measured the interval of time from the onset of the out- going sound to the arrival of the first echo. This can be done with a stop watch, provided that the hill is large enough and distant enough so that a clearly audible echo will return after some seconds. K the hill is too close, the time interval will be too short for easy and accurate measurement; if it is too far away, the echo may not be audible at all. Often there are too many hills producing multiple echoes, and if the first of these overlaps the end of the outgoing sound or there are reverberations from objects in our immediate vicinity, then the accurate measurement becomes difficult. Nor is it always easy to decide just which hill is sending back the echo; in fact, the easiest procedure often is to time the echo and then scrutinize a large-scale map in search of a steep hillside

57

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

at the correct distance. And there are ahnost always other sounds to compete for our attention. Thus obvious echoes have come to seem rather special sounds to be heard only in the most favorable circumstances.

One situation where echoes have been put to practical use is aboard boats in foggy coastal waters. Usually it is quiet in a fog and, aside from the boat itself, no echoing surface interrupts until the shore is reached. Often fishermen who find themselves in foggy waters and think that steep shore lines or cliffs may be within a mile or so produce a clear echo by making a short, loud sound. Sometimes this is a blast of a horn or whistle, required by law in any case to signal their presence to other boats, or the probing signal may be simply a shout. Some fishermen say they can even hear echoes from channel-marker buoys (about three feet in diameter) at several hundred feet. The usefulness of this method of navigation is often limited by the lack of adequate echoing targets in the air above the actual underwater hazards. Rocks need not reach the surface to be dan- gerous, and most shore lines are too gentle to provide reliable echoes.

Modem instruments have largely supplanted air- borne sound by transposing the same basic process into the water itself. Sound waves are broadcast from the boat's hull, and echoes of underwater sound from the bottom or from shoals ahead of the boat are recorded by instruments. Such devices for echolocation under water are called echo sounders or fathometers— ih& more refined models can even detect schools of fish. All these methods have in common the emission of a probing sound, the detection of echoes, and, most important, the discerning of the distance and direction of the object that returns the echo.

58

AIRBORNE ECHOES

The Acoustics of Clicks and Echoes

Because almost every object reflects sound to an ap- preciable degree, it is very rare for any sound to reach our ears without embellishment by echoes. Why then are the echoes so rarely noticed? Seldom do they occur separately; that is, they rarely arrive at a different time from the sound that produced them. Usually they and the original sound are mixed, and we ordinarily fail to discriminate between the two classes of sound waves. The simple experiments with a portable radio or tape recorder suggested in the previous chapter demonstrated that echoes were present indoors and that they could make a tone or a noise sound different. The loudness is increased by the addition of strong echoes from the walls of a room and by standing waves that may be audible when continuous pure tones are present indoors. But the important point is that special experiments were neces- sary to convince us that echoes really are so common a part of the most famihar sounds. One major reason echoes escape our notice so completely is the relatively long duration of most sounds compared to the time they require to bounce back in our customary places of Uving and hstening. Even on the shore of a moun- tain lake we are not Hkely to notice echoes of the songs we may sing about a campfire, for they will usually be masked by the notes that follow. Only when the song comes to an abrupt ending will the echoes from the hills intrude upon our consciousness. The masking of echoes by the continuing sound explains much of our inability to notice them in ordinary circumstances.

But all sounds come to an end, eventually at least, and there are always pauses or brief intervals of silence. Why don't we hear the echoes then? Suppose we try to investigate the physics of this question by setting up a

59

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

sensitive microphone to convert the energy of sound waves into electric voltages. Suppose further that we have connected this microphone to an instrument such as a cathode-ray oscilloscope, which draws a visible graph of the sound waves almost instantaneously. A cathode-ray oscilloscope is the forerunner of your tele- vision set. Inside the picture tube a spot of Ught is created, and one electric circuit moves this spot horizon- tally and again and again at a uniform rate from left to right while another moves it up and down. In this appli- cation the up-and-down movement is produced by the amplified voltage from the microphone. On the picture screen the combined horizontal and vertical motion lit- erally draws a graph of sound pressure agamst time.

With such a machine we can watch the behavior of the sound waves while we utter them. If we suddenly stop talking, the movement of the spot of Hght on the picture tube may seem to stop at the same instant. But if one looks closely, and if the instrument is set up in a large hall, one can easily see that the oscilloscope con- tinues briefly to draw a diminishing graph of the sound waves that are still traveling back and forth past the microphone from wall to wall. Because sound waves travel about 344 meters per second, and because less than 100 per cent of the sound energy is reflected back from each contact with the walls or floor, and finally be- cause sound waves are reduced slowly by their frictional effects on the molecules of the air, the continuing echoes are appreciable for only a fraction of a second. But they are there, and our eyes can see them on the oscilloscope screen even when our ears do seem not to hear them.

With instruments we can improve upon the ability of our eyes to judge how fast the sound level decUnes and how long it is detectable at all. One of the simplest methods is to photograph the moving spot on the oscillo-

60

AIRBORNE ECHOES

scope screen with a camera in which the fihn moves at a constant rate. While the spot moves up and down as the sound waves strike the microphone, the motion of the fihn draws a graph of sound pressure on the vertical axis against time on the horizontal axis. The resulting graphic picture of sound waves makes it easy to see the echoes which continue to arrive at the microphone a

Fig. 6. A graph of the sound pressure in a very short word without any echo is shown in A, and the same word with echoes is shown in B. Note the similarity of the early waves in A and B, and a difference as the echo returns before the original word has ended.

good fraction of a second after the end of the sound that came directly from a speaker's mouth. Such photographs also show clearly the greater magnitude of the echoes that follow the same word spoken indoors rather than out. An example of this comparison is seen in Fig. 6, but

61

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

even here where the word was a short one the echoes be- gan to mix with the original sound waves long before the end of the word.

Despite all these differences in the photographic por- trait, the same word spoken in these two situations sounds about the same. Again the amazing fact is that all these special procedures are necessary to convince us that the two sets of sound waves are not exactly the same. We have no difficulty recognizing the word or in telling who said it; it really does not sound very different in the living room from on the sidewalk. Why not? An- other simple experiment with a tape recorder throws considerable light on this question. If we place the tape recorder in an ordinary room (or, better still, a fairly large hallway or schoolroom with hard walls) and make a recording of a short sharp sound, we can play it back and hear it rather faithfully reproduced. Let us suppose that such a recording includes several repetitions of short words ending in hard consonants, such as bit, took, sud, or leg. Sharp clicks such as one can make by snap- ping together a pair of large scissors or pUers may also be used; and if one wishes a good excuse for it, a cap- pistol report is excellent for this experiment. In any event each recorded sound should be separated from the next by a few seconds of quiet.

When such a tape recording has been made, play it backward. That is, interchange the two reels so that the tape moves back end first when the machine is playing back the recording. What used to be the take-up reel becomes the reel from which the tape is unwound and vice versa. It will only be necessary on many recorders to turn the two reels upside down when interchanging them, so that the same side of the tape will pass next to the recording head. On some machines only half the width of the tape is magnetized when a recording is

62

AIRBORNE ECHOES

made, and when played back in the reverse direction, this side will not pass by the pick-up head. In this case the tape must be reversed so that the shiny side rather than the dull is next to the head. This reduces the level of the sound, but the volume control can usually be turned up to compensate for this loss, and the experi- ment can still be performed, though less well than with tape which is recorded across its full width.

When the tape is played backward, the echoes that followed the original word or click will of course pre- cede it. Since they were hardly noticeable before, one would naturally expect them to be a faint prelude to the reversed sound. But the actual result is a startling in- crease in the apparent loudness of the echoes. A click that sounded very sharp in its original form, or when a tape recording of it is played back in the normal direc- tion, now becomes a gradually rising hiss that culminates in the cUck. The cUck proper does not sound very dif- ferent frontward or backward, but the reversed echoes are much more apparent. So much so that when one hears this demonstration it is difl&cult to beUeve that the mstru- ment has not played some trick, that the whooossschk! is is really the same sound as the sharp cHck that gave rise to it.

This reversed playback technique reveals the real magnitude of the echoes from various sounds, but it is more difi&cult to appreciate with reversed speech or mu- sic, which sounds very abnormal in other ways. Clicks or pistol shots are in themselves so short that they con- tain only a few irregular sound waves, which are not very different-sounding when played in either direction. This can be demonstrated by repeating the recording out of doors in a quiet area well away from any large building. The clicks will now be accompanied by only minor echoes from the ground or other small objects,

63

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

such as trees or bushes. When played back in the reverse direction, they will sound far closer to the original than they did indoors. In short, this experiment shows the extent to which our sense of hearing de-emphasizes echoes. Sound waves which would be clearly audible if they existed in isolation are almost totally ignored if they happen to be part of an echo arriving a few tenths of a second after another sound. This goes far to explain why spoken words or other sounds do sound nearly the same when heard indoors with strong echoes from the walls or out of doors with few echoes or none. Of course there is a difference if one listens carefully for it, and, in addition to being louder, speech heard in a closed room has a "thicker" quaUty. The echoing sound of footsteps in a very large empty room is a common observation. Almost everyone has also noted the forlorn sounds of footsteps or conversation in a house emptied of its fur- niture and draperies. All these effects are caused by either the presence or absence of strong echoes.

The mechanism by which we suppress echoes is one of many subtle mysteries of the human ear and brain, and no one understands how it is accompUshed. The suppression lasts only a small fraction of a second; in- deed, it has been shown to be greatest immediately after the end of the direct sound and then to diminish grad- ually until after half a second or so another sound can be heard about as well as ever. An echo from a distant hillside arriving four or five seconds after the end of the outgoing sound is easy enough to hear if it is quiet where one is listening. But the same strength of echo would be inaudible if it arrived 1/lOth second after its original was emitted. By playing a tape recording backward, we remove the echoes from the time interval when our sup- pressor mechanism is at work.

In trying to learn what echoes sound hke, it is best to

64

AIRBORNE ECHOES

use sounds of short duration simply because they are less likely to overlap and be wholly masked by their originals. The sounds of spoken syllables or choking scissors are not as short as one would ideally Uke to use. Any sound shorter than about 1/lOth second is usually called a cUck, and the shorter it is the sharper it sounds, provided it has a reasonably high energy level. The hu- man voice cannot produce really short cUcks, however, nor can any other ordinary sound source. An electric spark caused by the discharge of a condenser is a very sharp cUck, and the discharge of a condenser through a loudspeaker is nearly as sharp, provided that the elec- trical circuit involved does not resonate and prolong the vibration of the speaker diaphragm. A cheaper and more widely available source of sharp clicks is a common toy, the frog or cricket made of a thin strip of spring steel with a dent in the middle. This is clamped tightly to a holder at one end; the other end is free to be pushed back and forth in such a way that the strip is bent and unbent. When your finger bends the strip of steel, the dent is suddenly inverted to impart a very sudden and energetic push to the air as it snaps from concave to convex or vice versa. The result is a very loud and sharp click, painfully loud if generated close to the ear, pos- sibly even damaging if it were to be repeated many times immediately in front of the ear opening.

The actual duration of the cUck varies from one model of toy to another, and it is affected by the size and shape of the holding device. In small clickers that I have tested the sound falls to 1/lOth its initial maximum within about 10 miUiseconds after the steel dent has snapped from one position to another. Recalling that the velocity of sound in air is approximately one foot per miUisecond, you can calculate that a click lasting 10 miUiseconds has a physical length of about 10 feet

65

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

as it travels through the air. This means that echoes will begin to reach the listener after being reflected from a wall 5 feet away just as the last of the cMck leaves the device. If we had a cUcker that gave out a 1 -millisecond sound, this overlap between echo and original sound would cease at distances greater than 6 inches.

It is interesting to take such a clicker and listen for its echoes. Even the ordinary toys producing 10- millisecond chcks will add significantly to the knowl- edge we obtained with echoes from spoken words. In these experiments it wiU be important to maximize the audibihty of the echoes while reducing the level of the outgoing sound which reaches our ears directly. Part of the echo-suppressing effect mentioned earlier is a very brief reduction in the sensitivity of our hearing for a fraction of a second after the arrival of a very large sound, and these clickers at close range are really very loud indeed. A good procedure is to hold a typical toy clicker with your two hands cupped around it and opened to form a forward facing horn so that the hands are between clicker and your ears. The outgoing chck will still be plainly audible, but its main sound energy output will be directed straight ahead. All the striking ef- fects I have described can be heard on reversed playback of such cUcks. In making a tape recording for reversed playback, you should keep the microphone behind the cupped hands, too, so that it also will be better situated to receive the echoes than to receive the original emitted click. With this very short click we can also begin to hear echoes directly without any tape recorder or reversed playback.

One of these toy clickers held in the cupped hands can be used to good advantage out of doors. If the hands and clicker are pointed at a building 50 feet or so away, a clear and separate echo can easily be heard.

66

AIRBORNE ECHOES

It can also be used to get distinct echoes from trees a foot in diameter, and other objects can be located in the same way. A good technique for a beginner is to sweep slowly back and forth with the clicker while operating it at a rate of one or two clicks per second. A few min- utes of careful Ustening will show that much can be learned about objects of this general size, provided that they are at a sufficient distance to yield an echo which is clearly separate from the emitted cHck itself. Experi- ence will show that echoes are most easily recognized when only one large echoing surface is within range. Several trees in a courtyard surrounded by large build- ings give multiple echoes that only careful scanning can resolve.

Before very long your hands become cramped from the unnatural position in which they must be held in order both to operate the cUcker and provide it with a horn. It is not difficult to mount the clicker in a small horn made of cardboard, Ught metal, or plastic. While a paraboUc shape is perhaps ideal, a fairly deep cone will serve fairly weU. The most important point is to provide a means of bending the dented sheet of steel back and forth without having any opening at the back of the horn through which the cUck can reach the user's ears directly at a high level of intensity. One device of this sort is shown in Fig. 7.

After you have learned to detect trees and houses by hearing their echoes, you will find it worth while to ex- periment with an easily recognized target such as a build- ing. Keep moving closer as you click. If you find it diffi- cult to be sure whether you are really hearing echoes, it may be helpful to try using the chcker while blindfolded or with your eyes closed. You will then be in much the same situation as a blind man trying to find his way about by means of echoes. Many blind people have

67

TRIGGER IMBEDDED IN RUBBER TO ALLOW DENTED STRIP OF STEEL TO BE BENT-

^

Fig. 7. A very satisfactory device for echo experiments can be made like this. The inside of the horn should be a paraboloid of revolution, and the clicker must be mounted at the focal point of the parabola. The Fiber- glas and plastic boat- or car-patching materials laid on a plaster of Paris form make excellent horns, and so do the parabolic reflectors of certain desk lamps.

68

AIRBORNE ECHOES

learned to do this with great skill and success. As you walk toward a building from 15-25 meters away, the echo of the cUcker is at first clearly separated from the original cHck but gradually merges with it until there is only one sound as best you can tell. At this point you should turn in some other direction, where no large ob- ject will return echoes, and operate the clicker several times. The clicks will sound different, and if in doubt you can alternately point toward the building and then in some other direction. After this difference has been recognized, you can move in closer to the building, re- peatedly clicking both toward it and away in non-echoing directions. It is surprising how close you can come and still be clearly aware of a difference in the sound of the clicker when it is pointed toward and away from the wall. At very close range, less than 10 feet for example, the difference will begin to be one of loudness; the echoes are of sufi&cient intensity that they add apprecia- bly to the click with which they are fused. This is why the horn is so important to shield you from the direct sound; if the horn could be perfect, so that all the sound energy of the clicker traveled away from you, then the echoes would become unmistakable.

It is helpful to digress at this point into a little thought about the wave lengths of audible sounds and the relationship of these wave lengths to the practicable size for a horn to direct the click forward. It is a general property of wave motion that specular (that is, mirror- like) reflections can be obtained only from objects that are larger than one wave length. Water waves on the surface of a ripple tank or a bathtub can be reflected from the edges of the tank or tub or from objects several centimeters long. Such reflections obey the same rules as those that hold for light waves; for instance, the angle of reflection from a plane surface equals the angle of

69

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

incidence. But quite different results are observed if the object reflecting the waves is only one wave length or less. Then one sees secondary waves which may be called echoes radiating in many directions from the small ob- ject. The strength of the echo waves in different direc- tions varies in a complicated way, both with the shape of the object and particularly with its size, relative to the wave length. In fact, if the object is much smaller than one wave length, its shape makes almost no differ- ence at all. Later on I shall describe some simple experi- ments with the clicker by which one can see how these same rules apply to audible sound waves. When the ech- oes travel in many directions from an object which itself is small compared to the wave length, they are often called scattered rather than reflected sound.

But we started this digression to consider how the wave length of the click would affect the usefulness of a horn to direct the sound straight forward. A horn is a special kind of acoustic mirror, and for this purpose we want one shaped so that sound waves generated some- where inside will all be reflected from the horn's surfaces, reinforcing each other and coming out of the mouth as parallel wave fronts traveling in the same direction. If the sound is generated at a point, the most effective horn to concentrate the sound waves into one direction will be one with a parabolic shape. This means that if you cut the horn longitudinally, any section will be a parabola with the sound source at its focus. One of the geometrical properties of a parabola is that any line radiating from the focus will strike the surfaces of the parabola at such an angle that when reflected (at an angle equal to the angle of incidence) it will be parallel to the axis of the parabola.

This sounds rather complicated, but perhaps Fig. 8 will help to make it clear. Really this is a very familiar

70

/

/

/

1

\

\ \

>

\

\ \

i

\ \

\

. 1

\

1

p*^

.,

\

\

y\

^

N

\

■■■

\ \

^-.

*

1

\

c

1

\

1 1

j

; 1

i f

/

/J

1

1

1

1

/

/

^<j

/

1

hz ^^^

.

/

\ 1

/

1

/■ ' 1

/ /

\ / / « .1

>

/

/

^^

. .--^

y%

^,,^-'''

.-^

Fig. 8. When the wave length is larger than the mouth of the horn, as in the low-frequency sound waves A^, Ag, and Ag, there is little or no focusing. But with a much smaller wave length a narrow beam of plane waves is produced.

71

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

Story, for searchlights, flashlights, and automobile head- lights are all made more or less according to this prin- ciple. But one of the important assumptions we have made in this line of reasoning is that the sound waves generated at the focus of a paraboUc horn really would be reflected from the surface of the horn at an angle equal to the angle of incidence. This is true only if the wave length is short compared to the size of the reflecting surface. K the wave length is much longer than the dimensions of the horn, very little direction will be im- parted to the sound waves. This means that a horn must be several wave lengths in size to do what we want it to do. What does this tell us about the frequencies of sound that should be produced by an echo-generating clicker?

Suppose we decide to use 256 sound waves per sec- ond. Since the velocity of sound is 344 meters per sec- ond, this frequency corresponds to a wave length of 344/256, or about 1.3 meters. To be effective, our horn must be several wave lengths in size, and even if it were made of the Ughtest possible materials it would be un- duly bulky. Clearly, then, we want short wave lengths or high frequencies. But we cannot go to frequencies above the upper limits of human hearing, which is somewhere between 15,000 and 20,000 c.p.s. A good compromise is about 5000 to 10,000 c.p.s. A wave whose frequency is 10,000 c.p.s. has a wave length of 344/10,000 meters, or a little under 3 centimeters. It is quite practicable to build and carry a horn sev- eral centimeters in size, and if this were the only consideration we would choose the highest frequencies or shortest wave lengths that were easily audible. Bats use frequencies up to 130,000 c.p.s. with wave lengths down to 2.5 millimeters, and their tiny mouths or ears can concentrate these short sound waves quite effec-

72

AIRBORNE ECHOES

tively. The toy clicker produces a number of frequencies or wave lengths within each brief click, but it would re- quire much more compUcated click generators to pro- duce an ideal click containing only a single frequency and a pulse short enough in duration to yield echoes distinctly separate from the original. Indeed, this con- sideration of separateness itself imposes limits on the possible frequencies. Several waves are necessary to es- tablish a clear frequency, and if our sound is to last only 1 millisecond it can contain only 10 waves of 10,000 c.p.s., or 5 waves of 5000 c.p.s.

What I have been suggesting in these simple experi- ments with a clicker is to act as though you were blind and see what you can discover about the larger objects in your surroundings solely by means of echoes. Later on I shall discuss in more detail what blind people actually do and the success they have achieved as well as the limitations that seem to prevent echolocation from warning them about aU the major obstacles that threaten their safe progress. But before turning to this direct apphcation to a pressing problem of a large group of handicapped persons, we will find it helpful to consider certain physical properties of echoes that de- termine their strength and audibility. For this purpose we can make good use of both real echoes from a clicker and "echoes" in the ripple tank, which is so useful in the physics laboratory for the analysis of wave motion.

The Velocity of Sound Measured by Means of Echoes

As a beginning we may consider a simple method of determining the approximate velocity of sound by an ex- tension of the already-mentioned procedure of timing the return of an echo from a distant hillside. If the distance

73

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

to the hill is not known and if the travel time of the sound and its echo is a few seconds, a good stop watch (which can measure time to a tenth of a second) would allow us to determine the distance to the hill, if we as- sume that we know the velocity of sound. Or if we know the distance, we can use the same time measurement to estimate the velocity at which the sound waves travel. If the basic limit of accuracy in our time measurement is determined by the stop watch at 0.1 second, the un- certainty in our measurement of distance would be the distance over which sound travels in that interval of time, or approximately 34 meters. But this would be the round-trip distance, so that theoretically we could measure the distance to the hill with an accuracy of ±17 meters. Another uncertainty is the human reaction time, the interval between the actual arrival of a sound and the pressing of the button on the stop watch. While this is an appreciable fraction, certainly more than 0.1 of a second, there is not likely to be a great difference between the first reaction time to the original sound and the reaction time in stopping the watch when one hears the echo; hence they will nearly cancel each other. An- other error is likely to occur if the emitted sound and the echo build up gradually. If a half second is needed to reach maximum sound intensity, and if the echo is enough fainter so that only the peak value is audible, then we will probably find that the stop watch is pressed one reaction time after the very beginning of the outgoing sound, but not until one reaction time after the echo is nearly at its peak. This can easily cause an error of about 0.3 second unless a very sharp sound is used for the ex- periment.

A similar experiment can be performed with the clicker, provided it can be operated fairly rapidly. Sup- pose you stand 30 meters from a large building and

74

AIRBORNE ECHOES

point the clicker so that a distinct echo is heard. Since the sound travels 60 meters from clicker to building and back to your ears, this trip will require 60/344, or about 0.17 second. If you operate the clicker twice per second, you will hear an outgoing chck at a time you may designate as zero, an echo at 0.17 second, a second emitted cUck at 0.50 second, a second echo at 0.67 sec- ond, etc. If we speed up our operation of the clicker, the second click will eventually come at 0.17 second and so will mask the echo. If we can operate the clicker with sufficient regularity, this fusion of echo with second click provides another way to measure distance— pro- vided we know the velocity of sound. A mechanical de- vice such as a metronome can control the rate of click- ing more precisely, but with a little practice a good approximation can be achieved. One practical difficulty is that the click made by bending the strip of steel will usually be slightly louder or different in quality from that made when the strip is unbent. Thus successive clicks alternate in level or quahty, and it is not always easy to maintain an even rhythm. But it can be done and, regardless of its practicability, it is worth while to understand this simple method for estimating distance by the rate of clicking necessary to cause each echo to fuse with the following click. One effective way to es- timate the critical rate is to have someone else count the number of clicks in a 5- or 10-second period measured with a stop watch or the second hand of an ordinary watch.

The same cUcker may also be used to demonstrate convincingly the concentration of echoes into certain directions when they have been reflected from surfaces of various sizes relative to the wave lengths in the click. Most toy clickers have a frequency range between 3 and 10 kilocycles, so that the most intense soimd waves have

75

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

wave lengths of a few centimeters. When such wave lengths strike the wall of a building, they are reflected almost exactly as Hght waves would be from a mirror. If the clicker is pointed directly at the wall, the echo will come straight back, but if the emitted sound strikes ob-

Zdab= Zeac

Fig. 9. The law of reflection describes the way in which sound reflects from a large flat surface. When making this experiment, observe the relative positions of the two boys.

liquely, the echoes will rebound away from the clicker, as indicated in Fig. 9. This is why it is so easy to locate a building by scanning with the clicker; the echo is far louder when the horn is pointed straight at the wall. Two people can co-operate in a simple experiment that dem-

76

AIRBORNE ECHOES

onstrates how these echoes behave. One should aim the clicker at the wall 20° to 30° to one side of a perpen- dicular from clicker to wall, while the second listens for the echo. He will not hear it so clearly if he stands be- side the clicker at point B as he will if he walks to one side and a Uttle behind the clicker to a point such as C. The position where the echo is loudest can be pre- dicted on the same principle that governs the specular reflection of light from mirrors; namely, that the angle of reflection, r (angle EAC), equals the angle of incidence, / (angle DAB). This experiment will give clearer results if the listener stands a little behind the clicker, so that he is shielded from the direct, outgoing click by the horn. The same experiment can be performed more accurately by mounting the clicker on a camera tripod and turning it slowly to different angles relative to the wall. The lis- tener may then move back and forth until he finds the points where he hears the echo most clearly. Or the Us- tener may stand still in various positions while the first person turns the clicker slowly back and forth from right to left according to his instructions. It is remarkable how well the results of such experiments confirm the rule that the angle of reflection equals the angle of incidence.

There is an entirely different situation in which it is easy to experience a simple type of echolocation. When you ride in an automobile, sitting by an open window, you hear a number of soimds from the engine, the tires, and the rush of air past the window. As you drive past a high stone wall, through an underpass, or close to any large surface, these sounds will change in quality. A se- ries of concrete guardrail posts, the masonry posts used to support iron fences, or even a row of wooden fence posts can be detected from a rapid series of swishing sounds as the car moves by. Try Ustening with your eyes closed as you ride along some familiar route and you

77

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

may be surprised to find how many places you can rec- ognize by ear. If you find a series of clearly "audible" fence posts, compare their sound effects with those you hear in passing through an underpass. Along the posts it is primarily the high frequencies that return as echoes from the relatively small surfaces; in the underpass al- most the whole range of sounds of the car will be re- flected from the large wall surface. K you make a care- ful study of these sounds while your car is driven at about the same speed, you will find that you can learn to recognize many types of structures, such as parked cars, from the echoes which they add to the roughly con- stant sounds made by your own car.

Echoes are used by bats and men to locate smaller and more elusive objects than the walls of buildings, and some interesting properties of reflected waves be- come important once we begin to work with smaller ob- jects. After you have acquired some experience with the chcker, it is of interest to try it on trees, telephone poles, or other objects that can easily be found out in the open away from other echo-making objects. With care and practice you can detect trees as small as 6 inches from several feet away, and when this has been accomphshed, you can again call upon another person to point the clicker at the tree while you, the listening observer, move about to different positions to find where the echo sounds loudest. The result will usually be that the echo can be heard over a much wider range of angles than hap- pened with the louder echo from a building. This is because the tree is only a Httle more than one wave length in diameter and the echoes are spread over a much wider range of directions, as indicated in Fig. 10. Just as a horn less than one wave length in size fails to concentrate sound, small objects scatter their echoes. If you can hear echoes from trees or poles as small

78

''■N

_^

-->.

s

/ / /

7-'

^— --,

/

/

_^,— -

A ■'

TREE TRUN-K

I !

*•-.

\ N \ ^ / /

*

Ffg. 76^. fF/ien the wave length is greater than the size of the object {here a tree trunk), the echo, or scattered sound moves out in all directions. The solid lines indi- cate the original sound, the dashed lines the echoes, and the width of the lines the intensity of sound.

79

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

as one or two wave lengths, you will find them al- most equally loud over a wide angular range. Of course they are nowhere as loud as those from larger structures such as buildings. This would be just as true of light waves or water waves, and an appropriate experiment in the ripple tank will show specular reflec- tion of surface waves from long objects but would show extensive scattering from something about one wave length in size.

This difference between specular reflection and scat- tering of waves can be studied with a ripple tank or even with the surface waves in a bathtub, although it is more difficult to see them clearly in the tub. Just as echoes are easier to hear if generated by sounds of short duration, it is easier to study surface echoes by generating short trains or pulses of water waves. This is probably why water beetles interrupt their swimming motions at fre- quent intervals— to provide intervals of "quiet" in which they can better feel the echoes from objects at some dis- tance across the water's surface. If one sets up a few sur- face waves at a time by a quick light tap against the water, reflections from the edge of the tank or tub are of course easy to see. If an object of about one wave length (for example, a short piece of broomstick or wooden dowel) is placed in the water with its axis per- pendicular to the surface, close observation will find smaller waves scattering out in almost all directions from this source of surface echoes. Of course all other waves must be absent, but, once this phenomenon has been ob- served, it is of some interest to vary the size of the cy- lindrical object from the smallest that produces visible scattered waves up to sizes well in excess of one wave length. Such experiments convince one of the real dif- ference between sharply directional specular reflection and the diffuse scattering from small echo sources. These

80

AIRBORNE ECHOES

two general types of echoes will be important when we move on from the physics of echoes to a study of the actual uses to which they are put by blind men and by the bats and other animals which have developed such refined and precise methods of echolocation for the carrying out of their daily business.

81

CHAPTER 4 The Language of Echoes

From our brief, qualitative look at the remarkable navi- gational feats of some animals, it seemed clear that sound was a most important message carrier. This led us to a detailed examination of sound itself, particularly how it echoes or reflects, in order that we might experiment more skillfully and intelligently in an attempt to learn how echoes are actually used by animals— what are their limits, what aids or hinders, what physical characteristics are especially suited to echolocation, what are the special characteristics of the sounds these animals make. We may hope to discover some important bits of evidence, perhaps obscure at the moment, which will aid blind peo- ple in their travels, and even if this does not occur, we will certainly know our environment better. Men have al- ways learned from animals, and even in this age of elec- tronics and atomic structure we still have much to learn. Since the bats are so expert in the use of echoes, let us be- gin by examining in more detail the sounds they broad- cast to produce the echoes which guide their agile flight.

83

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

Orientation Sounds of Bats

Bats make a variety of vocal sounds; for example, when disturbed they squeak and chitter. But we are in- terested primarily in the sounds they use in flight to gen- erate useful echoes that tell them about objects at some distance. These orientation sounds are all of high fre- quency, though they overlap slightly the range of human hearing to produce the very faint audible ticking we have discussed. But most of the sound energy emitted by fly- ing bats lies at frequencies from 10 to 150 kc in different species, and I will describe only one or two examples of orientation sounds that have been measured from a few typical kinds of bats.

One of the simplest acoustic patterns is that used by the horseshoe bats, an insectivorous group that lives in Europe, Asia, Austraha, and Africa. They use orienta- tion sounds of nearly a single frequency, which may be anywhere from 60 to 120 kc, depending on the species. The individual sounds last only a small fraction of a second, usually from 50 to 100 miUiseconds, but this is much longer than the duration of other bats' sounds. The name horseshoe bat refers to a complicated se- ries of folds or membranes surrounding the nostrils and the mouth with two roughly concentric rosettes which vaguely resemble a horseshoe when viewed from in front. The German zoologist Franz P. Moehres has shown recently that the horseshoe serves as a small horn to concentrate the emitted sound into a narrow beam which is swept back and forth as the bat scans its sur- roundings. Bats have a habit of hanging head downward by the hind feet, and the horseshoe bats have especially flexible hip joints. They can pivot through almost a complete circle as they scan with their beam of high-

84

THE LANGUAGE OF ECHOES

frequency sound. Often they dart out from such a posi- tion to seize an insect that flies within range.

Another group of bats, confined to the tropics, feed mostly upon fruit, but some also eat insects, which they may pick off the vegetation. These bats emit much fainter sounds than the horseshoe bats— ex- tremely brief clicks, lasting from a fraction of a milU- second to 2 or 3 milliseconds. The sound waves making up these very short pulses are compUcated, with a va- riety of frequencies from as low as 10 to as high as 150 kc, again depending on the species. The vampire bats, which feed on the blood of Uving animals and men, be- long to this group. Without causing the victim to awake from his sleep, they feed by making small cuts with their very sharp teeth and drinking the blood that flows for a time before clotting. All these bats seem to refrain from the active pursuit of flying insects, and the intensity level of their sounds is so low that only the best of micro- phones and sound-analyzing equipment will register them. They may be called whispering bats to distinguish them from the other two groups.

The third major category includes the common in- sectivorous bats that are well known in North America and Europe. With a very few exceptions, these bats all hunt flying insects in the open, tracking their elusive moving prey on the wing, maneuvering through com- plicated spUt-second turns and other acrobatics to follow and intercept the erratic flight of moths and flying beetles, May flies and mosquitoes. The sounds used by this group, only a few miUiseconds in duration and al- most as intense as those of the horseshoe bats, have a characteristic frequency pattern. Each orientation soimd starts at a very high frequency and drops rapidly during its brief life, to end about an octave below the frequency at which it started. The common little brown bats of the

85

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

Fig. 11. The frequency and wave length of a bat's sound vary during each chirp. This diagram, which is approximately to scale, illustrates the small amount of sound reflected by one insect.

United States begin each of their orientation sounds at about 90 kc and end at 45 kc. Since each sound lasts only about 2 milliseconds, this is a very rapid change in frequency; indeed, this bat sweeps through a frequency band double the whole range of the human ear in 2

86

THE LANGUAGE OF ECHOES

milliseconds. As illustrated in Fig. 11, a typical orienta- tion sound contains only about 50 sound waves, no two exactly alike. The wave length of the initial waves is only half the wave length of those making up the end of the emitted sound. These sounds are chirps, at least that is what we call audible sounds made by certain in- sects when they sweep through as wide a range of fre- quencies within a fraction of a second. This type is sometimes called a frequency-modulated pulse of sound, and this group of bats may be thought of as "FM bats" in contrast to the horseshoe bats with their much longer, sharply beamed tones of nearly constant frequency, and the faint but complex cHcks of the tropical "whispering bats."

Echoes of Insect Prey

The FM or chirping bats have been studied much more thoroughly than the other two groups; therefore, more is known about them. They seem to be the most highly specialized for a life of flight, very expert at ma- neuvering under the most difl&cult conditions. The daily (or nightly) business of catching insect food compels them to be highly skilled in the detection of such small moving objects and in the aerial acrobatics necessary to intercept them. Since bats do almost all their hunting on dark nights, often approaching insects from above or in wooded areas where they would have to be seen against a dark background, visual detection must be impossible. And SpaUanzani, as we have said in Chapter 1, showed before 1800 that blind bats catch as many insects as normal animals. It has usually been thought that they located insects by listening for the sounds of their wing- beats, and this probably does occur in some circum- stances when the flying insects make appreciable hum-

87

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

ming or buzzing noises. But I have discovered in recent years that the orientation sounds, the high-frequency chirps of these bats, are repeated at remarkably high rates as the bats locate and close in upon flying insects. Furthermore, bats will often pursue imitation insects such as pebbles or Uttle wads of wet absorbent cotton tossed gently into the air as they fly by. They do not actu- ally bite or swallow such decoys, but they swoop avidly towards them with the same increase in the tempo of the orientation sounds they employ when chasing real in- sects under natural conditions. When one realizes how silent are many of the small insects upon which bats feed, it becomes rather Ukely, though not rigorously proven, that the bats detect at least some of their insect prey by hearing echoes of their own chirps bouncing oQ the insects rather than relying solely on the sounds emitted by the insects themselves.

I shall return a Uttle later to the patterns in which these orientation sounds are broadcast under various conditions, including the pursuit of insect prey. But first let us consider the effectiveness of the process of insect himting. Just how many insects does a bat catch in a given time? How big are the insects caught? At what distances are they detected? Only very recently have we been able to provide even partial and tentative answers to such questions. Spallanzani and others who examined the stomachs of bats just returned from a night's hunting have marveled at the relatively large mass of finely chewed insect remains present in the digestive tract of every successful bat. One study showed that little brown bats weighing 7 grams commonly catch 1 gram of insects per hour of active hunting. Very recently we have been able to persuade a few bats to hunt insects in a laboratory flight room where the process could be studied and pho- tographed. One smaller relative of the little brown bat,

88

THE LANGUAGE OF ECHOES

weighing only 3.5 grams, caught mosquitoes at such a rapid rate before our very eyes that after 15 minutes' hunting its weight had increased by 10 per cent to 3.85 grams. These particular mosquitoes weighed about 0.002 gram each. The bat had no possible way of gain- ing weight during these 15 minutes of closely observed himting, aside from the weight of the mosquitoes caught. It drank no water and ate nothing else. It probably lost a little weight by the evaporation of water while breath- ing; therefore, it caught more than 0.35 gram of mos- quitoes.

Dividing the weight gain by the weight of a single mosquito shows that at least 175 mosquitoes were caught in 15 minutes, or more than one every 6 seconds. This was also approximately the number of obvious mosquito-chasing maneuvers that we could count dur- ing this hunting spree. There is every reason to believe that similar rates of insect capture are commonplace events in the nightly activities of millions of these bats and their relatives all over the world. Of course, it is not always mosquitoes that are eaten; almost any kind of insect that is locally available and is not too big seems welcome. Sometimes moths up to an inch in wingspread are taken, but at other times these bats capture insects much smaller than mosquitoes. In one instance a smaU gnat weighing only 0.0002 gram was found stiQ unswal- lowed in the mouth of a bat killed while it was hunting.

This maneuvering to capture one insect every 6 sec- onds is what makes the flight of bats appear so erratic. Far from being feeble fliers buffeted about by air cur- rents, they are expert fliers engaging in the difficult in- terception of flying insects. Their percentage of successes must be very high indeed. Certainly they are doing vastly better than simply flying around with their mouths open. Even when mosquitoes are particularly abundant, their

89

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

density is such that one of these small bats would have to fly all night before its mouth encountered a single mosquito purely by chance. Yet the actual rate of capture is one every few seconds.

Photographs of the bat pursuing an insect show that they sometimes begin their maneuvers when 2 or 3 feet from a mosquito. The pattern of the orientation sounds begins to change a fraction of a second before the bat turns toward its victim. The implications of these obser- vations can be understood after a brief explanation of the rate at which a bat's frequency-modulated chirps are repeated during various types of flight. When a Uttle brown bat is flying fairly straight and is not close to any- thing of immediate concern, it repeats its 1- to 2-milli- second chirps at rates of 10 to 20 per second. But when- ever it approaches any small obstacles, such as wires stretched across a laboratory room to test its skill, many more chirps are emitted in a given interval of time. For brief periods the repetition rate may rise as high as 250 per second. When this happens, the individual chirps be- come shorter, usually less than 1 millisecond, so that silent intervals still exist between chirps. When the high- frequency sounds of these bats are studied under natural conditions, a clear distinction between straight and level flight and the active pursuit of flying insects becomes ap- parent. Such eavesdropping is only possible, of course, when we have apparatus which will detect the bat sounds that are inaudible. In one convenient form this apparatus "translates" each of the bat's high-frequency sounds into audible clicks in earphones or a small loudspeaker. This makes it possible to watch the bat while at the same time "listening" to its orientation sounds in this trans- posed form. Most of the details, such as the octave of frequency modulation, are lost, but there is one cUck

90

THE LANGUAGE OF ECHOES

from the loudspeaker every time the bat broadcasts one of its high-frequency chirps.

When this "listening" apparatus is used in some spot where bats do their insect hunting, we notice that one cruising past on a straight course several feet above the ground sounds like the slow putt-putt-putt-putt of a lazy, old gasoline engine. Often it will fly straight past with Uttle or no change in this rhythm, but if its attention is attracted either to a real insect or to a decoy, such as a pebble tossed up in front of it, then there is a marked increase in the rate of the orientation sounds. Sometimes this is a slight increase in rate, but if the pursuit is serious, involving drastic maneuvers such as sudden turns, wing- overs, or sharp dives, then the translation resembles the acceleration of a motorcycle engine. On occasion it rises to a real crescendo with the individual clicks coming so close together that for human ears they fuse into a whin- ing buzz reminiscent of a chain saw. Such crescendos occur just when the bat is closing in on an elusive moving target, strong evidence against the idea that all location and tracking are done simply by listening to the sounds of the insect's wingbeats. In this case one would expect the bat to keep relatively quiet when near an insect so as to hear the faint buzzing of the insect's wings. Instead it fills the air with an extremely rapid series of chirps that would seem to interfere severely with any process of passive Ustening.

Precision of Echolocation

Another important aspect of bats' use of echoes for rapid and precise navigation is the small size of objects which can be detected and the distances at which detec- tion can occur. The only feasible tests yet devised have involved wires or strings rather than small isolated ob-

91

00

^

s

Co

!<

e

i

o

■^

•fc*

■»«*

a

e

C)

Q

o

Q

S

•S2

1

-5?

-«:

•K*

=^

■^

S

^

1

•S2

s

•s

K

'Kk

"T3

=1

■&;

^

s

^

-Q

.ik.

K

•12

^

^

S

"S

-^

to

^

?

se

?

sr

'*-,

•S

O

^

.op

<^

-s

.1

c

^

c

Co

(^

05

o

?

fN

s

•^

^

o

dr)

"Q

■§

:**

r

Uh

o

■a

92

THE LANGUAGE OF ECHOES

jects like insects or pebbles. It is simply too difficult to keep small particles stationary in the air long enough to make accurate tests of bats' ability to dodge them. When wires are strung across a laboratory "flight" room, on the other hand, as diagramed in Fig. 12, the animals seem anxious to avoid collisions, although the brown bats weigh so little that they do not seem to be injured even in an occasional head-on crash against a taut wire.

When the wires are spaced 30 centimeters apart, or sUghtly more than the wingspread of the Uttle brown bat, they make a difficult barrier that even the most skillful animals brush against lightly from time to time. The wires can be made smaller and smaller, without any marked effect on the percentage of misses registered by a really skillful animal, down to a wire diameter of a frac- tion of a millimeter. To be sure, many bats wlQ be found on first testing in such an obstacle course to be clumsy, striking even the larger wires, but this is usually because they are in poor condition or not completely awakened from the deep sleep into which they lapse even on sum- mer days. It is necessary to reduce the wire diameter to 0.07 millimeter (about the diameter of a human hair) before the little brown bats strike them at random. Even slightly larger wires, 0.12 millimeters in diameter, while difficult to miss, are dodged by the really skillful "ath- letes" among our experimental subjects far more often than one would expect.

Astonishing as it is that bats can detect wires as small as 0.12 millimeters, these previous experiments do not tell us at what distance this detection occurs. But motion pictures of the bats will give some indication of the range of detection when the translated orientation sounds are put on a sound track of the movie. Careful study of such movies, frame by frame, has enabled us to find the dis- tance at which the rate of repetition of the bats' chirps

93

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

first begins to increase. This varies from flight to flight, even for a single individual, but the average of numerous measurements with several sizes of wire gave the follow- ing results.

Average

Average

rep. rate

distance

Diameter

before

Average

at which rep.

of wires

approach

maximum

rate first

(miUi-

to wires

rep. rate

increases

meters)

(pulses/sec)

(pulses/sec)

(centimeters)

3.0

12

50

215

1.07

12

40

185

0.65

13

30

150

0.46

13

40

120

0.28

12

27

105

0.18

12

22

90

These distances are considerably greater than one would guess from the bat's flight behavior. Ordinarily it flies along a fairly straight course and swerves only in the last few inches to avoid a wire. Yet the increasing pulse rate shows that it has already detected the wires and reacted to them at the distances shown in the table. If no wire is in place, there is no increase in the rate of the orientation chirps. Of course, a bat might be aware of the wires at still greater distances than the table shows, but it gives no sign of such awareness that we yet have learned to recognize. The important point is that even such small wires as those 0.18 millimeters in diameter are detected at some distance, not merely at the last pos- sible moment to avoid colUsion. It is also interesting to note that small wires produce only a small increase in pulse rate. Actually the bat is moving so fast (ap-

94

THE LANGUAGE OF ECHOES

proximately 4 meters per second) that with the smaller wires it often has time for only 2 or 3 additional pulses above the number it would have employed had there been no wire in place. All these facts testify that the echolocation practiced by bats is a refined, accurate method of orientation, not merely a crude sort of gropmg.

Bread upon the Waters

Nor do these examples by any means exhaust the list of difficult tasks which bats accomplish with some aid at least from echolocation. Certain of the whispering bats catch insects, small birds, or lizards that are resting on vegetation, but we are not sure that they do this by means of echolocation. They may simply listen to characteristic sounds coming from their prey. More amazing is the fact that four different species of the FM bats make their liv- ing by catching fish. This they do by flying just above the surface of the water and every now and then dipping their hind feet just below the surface. The claws on these feet are long, curved, and sharp, and the bats manage to gaff small minnows often enough to fill their stomachs every evening (as shown in Fig. 13 ) . When fishing in this way on the darkest nights (and often with mist rising from the water) , they emit a rapid series of chirps much like those of their insect-catching relatives. The gap be- tween the two types of food gathering is not as great as it might at first seem, for the insectivorous bats drink by skimming the surface of the water and dipping their chins just deep enough to secure a drop of water at a time. This requires fine control, for a millimeter too deep would surely result in a dunking. These insectivorous bats also catch insects resting on the water surface, so perhaps it was a small step from this habit to reach for

95

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

fish below the surface. In any event, the fish-catching species make much of their living in this way, and during their recent evolutionary history a relatively small ana- tomical adaptation has resulted in the specialized fish- gafiSng claws.

When I have watched these bats in Panama, I have seen no sign that the fish were moving or disturbing the surface of the water in any way. Often it was glassy calm, and the bat flew for hundreds of feet a few inches above the surface, quickly lowering the hind feet into the water for a short distance and then raising them while continuing its low-altitude searching flight. How do these bats know where fish are to be captured? They are evi-

Fig. 13. Motion pictures of fishing bats actually gaf- fing minnows provided the model for this drawing. Prentice Bloedel took the photographs.

dently selective in their fishing, for they fly long distances just above the water and only rarely dip their claws be- neath the surface. Since the fishing occurs on dark and misty nights, it is most unlikely that the fish could be seen and still less probable that they emit any sound audible to the bat flying in the air above the surface. Could it be that the fish-catching bats detect echoes from fish beneath the surface? At first glance this may seem only a slight modification of the process by which closely related bats catch insects in the air. But the physical dis- continuity between air and water makes the transmission

96

THE LANGUAGE OF ECHOES

of sound difficult, and so echolocation seems an unlikely explanation.

As mentioned earlier in connection with the underwa- ter hearing of fish and porpoises, sound waves have great difficulty in passing from air to water or vice versa. When airborne sound impinges on a smooth surface of water, with its direction of travel perpendicular to the water surface, only 0.12 per cent of the energy of the airborne sound continues beneath the surface as underwater sound waves. For a sound wave travehng from water to air, the same small fraction of the acoustic energy strik- ing the surface from below continues outwards into the air. This means that a flying bat's orientation sounds striking the water, penetrating into it, being reflected back from a fish, and passing out into the air again would be reduced to (0.0012)2, or 1.44 x 10"^ of the original sound, during the two trips through the air- water interface. To this great reduction must be added further losses: only a small fraction of the emitted sound would be reflected by a fish, and only a small fraction of what did escape into the air would strike the ears of the listening bat. These figures make it seem almost hopeless for a bat to try to detect fish through the water surface by their echoes, but before dismissing the whole idea as utterly impossible, let us compare what insect-eating bats are known to do in air with the hypothetical location of fish by their echoes.

Certain of the FM bats are able to detect a pebble or a flying insect 1 centimeter in diameter from at least 200 centimeters away. At distances of more than about 10 centimeters from the bat's mouth the sound intensity falls off as the square of the distance. Since a 1 -centimeter insect is a small target, sound is scattered from it ap- proximately as it would be from a point source, so that the echo intensity also varies inversely as the square of

97

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

the distance. Therefore, as the distance from bat to insect increases, the net strength of the echo returning to the animal's ears falls off as the fourth power of the distance. If the insect is twice as far away, the intensity of its echo is 1/2*, or l/16th as great. Let us suppose, for the sake of argument, that a fishing bat does detect a small fish at a distance of 10 centimeters. Since a fish's body is acoustically similar to water, any echo it produces would be most likely to come from the swim bladder. This is an air-filled chamber which most small fresh-water fish have within their bodies, and in a minnow it would be about 1 centimeter, or the same size as the insects detected in air at 200 centimeters. If all other factors were equal, a target at 10 centimeters would return an echo stronger than one at 200 centimeters by a factor of (200/10)*, or 1.6 X 10^. The two trips through the water surface would reduce the fish echo by a factor of 1.33 X 10"^. The product of these two numbers is 0.23, which would mean that the echo received by the fishing bat under these hypothetical conditions would have about one fourth the intensity of the echo which is actually detected by the bat catching insects in air. If this is really a vaHd comparison, it puts the possibility of catching fish by echoes in quite another light, since a factor of four is well within the uncertainty of the assumptions I have made. For example, the insect-catching bat may well detect 1 -centimeter insects at more than 200 centimeters, and an increase in the distance of detection to 280 centi- meters would produce an echo from the insect equal to that from the hypothetical fish echo.

This numerical argument, however, does not prove that the fishing bats really do hear echoes from fish through the water surface. It simply means that this pos- sibility merits consideration and should not be rejected out of hand because of the very large energy loss during

98

THE LANGUAGE OF ECHOES

the two passages through the air-water interface. Or, to put the matter in another way, the detection of insects at 2 meters through the air means that bats are capable of hearing echoes roughly as faint as those that might, under ideal conditions, return from a minnow to a fish- ing bat. The book Listening in the Dark goes into this particular problem in more detail if you wish to pursue it further. But it is significant that a hypothesis which seemed so completely ridiculous when one first learned of the milUonfold loss of energy during the round trip from air to water should turn out, on closer examina- tion, to be a serious possibility after all. Common sense and first impressions may be misleading when we are dealing with matters quite outside the range of ordinary human experience upon which people have built what we call common sense.

Resistance to Jamming

Up to this point we have been thinking about echoes as more or less isolated sound waves that could be dealt with one at a time. To be sure, we considered earHer the likeUhood that a faint echo would be masked by the louder outgoing sound. Experiments described in Chap- ter 3 demonstrated that human hearing ignores echoes arriving within a small fraction of a second after a loud sharp chck. Bats obviously do better than we in dis- criminating these echoes from the original sound. In the experiments of Schevill and Lawrence a porpoise showed that it could detect echoes from a small fish despite the louder competing echoes from the bottom of the pond, the surface, and the shore a few feet beyond this small- sized target. But the expertness of bats goes even further than anything we have yet considered. When they are hunting insects, their ears receive a more complicated

99

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

mixture of sounds than merely their original chirp plus a single echo returning from a single insect and having the same wave form at a lower energy level. What really impinges upon their ears is a whole series of echoes from everything within several feet— the ground, other insects, and every bush, twig, tree trunk, leaf, or blade of grass. Many of these things contribute only small amounts of echo energy, but the echo from an insect is itself a faint one, and if it is audible so must the others be. How then do bats sort out one class of faint echoes from all the others? How do they hear the difference between echoes that mean food to be caught and those that mean ob- stacles to be dodged?

If we knew how bats discriminate so expertly be- tween faint insect echoes and the competing echoes ar- riving within a small fraction of a second, we could make more rapid progress toward solving the orientation problems of blind people, to say nothing of developing instruments that could emulate the bats more perfectly. Unfortunately this is not yet possible, but it is interesting to consider how well bats can make such discrimina- tions. This cannot be easy, even for a bat, and faint echoes from wire obstacles are less skillfully utilized when stronger echoes arrive in the same small fraction of a second. For example, we once performed an ex- periment in which two rows of wires were stretched across a flight room, one row at the middle of the room and the other row 45 centimeters from the end wall, as diagramed in Fig. 12. In both rows the wires extended from floor to ceiling and were spaced 30 centimeters apart. When the diameter of the wires was 0.46 milli- meter, they were difficult echo targets, but the percent- age of misses in a large number of flights through the central row was 91 per cent. This represents a considera- ble degree of success, and almost all the contacts were

100

THE LANGUAGE OF ECHOES

very light touches when the bat did not quite manage to time its wingbeats so that the wing tips cleared the obstacle.

When the same animals not only flew through the mid- dle row but also continued on through the end row, their success was much less— the percentage of misses fell from 91 per cent at the middle row to 58 per cent for the end row which was 45 centimeters from the end wall. This re- sult was probably due to the very much larger echo from the end wall. The situation can be understood in terms of Fig. 14, a schematic graph of the sound energy reaching the bat's ears during the fraction of a second when each chirp is emitted and its several echoes return. The upper graph (A) depicts the situation when the middle row of wires is being detected and avoided; the middle graph (B) applies to the same size of wire located 45 centi- meters from the end wall, while the third graph (C) de- scribes a further experiment in which the wires near the end wall were 1.07 millimeters in diameter. In C, the bat's success was about the same (88 per cent misses) as it had been with the 0.46 millimeter wires at the mid- dle of the flight room ( A) . Under natural conditions the important echoes would be those from an insect rather than a wire, and the competing echoes would arrive from many different objects, such as the ground, tree trunks, or branches of trees. These would produce more com- pHcated echoes than those from the end wall of the flight room and would be present over a longer period of time, but they would never include as strong a single echo as that from the large end wall. An approximation to this case is represented in the fourth graph (D) of Fig. 14, where it has been assumed that some of the extraneous echoes have come from objects closer than the insect it- self. This must happen when bats hunt, as they often do,

101

BAT'S ORIGINAL PULSE P

END WALL ECHO

E

WIRE ECHO W

m

fi^^tA^l^^^

Fig. 14. Schematic graphs of a bat's chirp and the echoes in the flight room {Fig. 12) under various test conditions: A— approaching 0.46-millimeter wires in the middle of room; B— approaching same wires near the end wall; C— approaching larger wires close to the end wall {note the larger echo); and D— approaching an insect under natural conditions in woods where many other objects also return echoes. {For simplicity the frequency modulation is not shown.)

102

THE LANGUAGE OF ECHOES

in thickly wooded areas where competing echoes ob- scure the important echo from the insect.

The success of bats in catching one insect every few seconds testifies to their ability not only to hear the in- sect echoes but to sort them out of a welter of other, competing echoes. This process has been studied in the laboratory by modifying the circumstances to standard- ize the conditions and permit measurements of the bats' performance. Rather than studying bats as they hunt insects in the woods, we generated artificial sounds in our flight room so that these noises were added to the echoes from wires, floor, and walls. In other words, we tried to confuse or "jam" the bats. The result was a sur- prising and revealing failure. The bats continued to dodge wires of 1 to 2 millimeters even in the most in- tense noise we could produce, a loud hissing that covered the whole frequency range of their orientation sounds. Skillful animals avoided wires of this size just as well in the noise as in the quiet, even though the noise was much louder than the echoes from the wires. These ex- periments could theoretically be shown as a fifth graph in Fig. 14, but an accurate representation of the noise would obUterate any representation of echoes from the wires.

There are limits, however, to the discriminating ability of even the most skillful bat. If the wires are made smaller and smaller, a size is finally reached where the echoes no longer can be detected. The smallest wire which can be detected in noise is greater than the small- est wire detectable in quiet. For one species of bat the minimum in the quiet was about 0.25 millimeter, and in the noise the minimum size increased to 0.5 to 0.7 millimeter, depending upon the individual animal and its condition at the time of the particular experiment.

103

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

(Listening in the Dark has a more detailed account of these experiments.)

What emerges from these several examples of orien- tation based on echoes is the simple fact that bats and porpoises are most adept at locating small and distant objects in this way. Furthermore, they do so with a pre- cision and acuity that are understandable only when one remembers that this is how they make their Uving. If a bat fumbles with its echoes, it goes hungry. Hunger is a powerful incentive, tending strongly to improve any mechanism or process subjected to this selective action. This is what biologists call natural selection, the process responsible for the evolution of plants and animals into their many diversified and complex forms. It is a slow process but an extremely effective one, and in the bats and porpoises we see the end result achieved through natural selection, perfecting over miUions of years the animals' faculties for utilizing echoes. Finally, it is im- portant to realize that the use of echoes requires the bats and porpoises to possess more than merely a means for generating sounds that in turn will yield echoes. It is also essential that these animals discriminate certain impor- tant echoes from complex mixtures of other sounds that are often much louder than those conveying the crucial information about food.

Discrimination of one portion of a complex sound from louder components is not a special skill of bats and porpoises. All animals endowed with a sense of hearing discriminate, and in many respects the human ear and brain are the best of all. When we listen to speech or music, we sort out a few significant portions of a compUcated mixture of shifting wave forms. If we hear people speaking an unknown foreign language, we receive a similar jumble of sound waves, but one to which we have no key. Footsteps or bat chirps and their

104

THE LANGUAGE OF ECHOES

echoes are a special language of their own. It is much simpler than German, Chinese, or English, but men, particularly blind men, find it very difficult to learn this language. Yet bats no larger than a baby mouse under- stand it well enough to catch ten mosquitoes every min- ute in the dark. What is it in a bat's tiny brain that per- mits understanding of this language and unlocks this library of useful information? No one yet knows the answer. We cannot even be sure we are asking the proper questions.

105

CHAPTER 5 Sonar and Radar

Although men have not learned the "language of echoes," they have been remarkably successful in de- signing echolocating instruments which surpass those of animals in many ways but remain quite inferior in other respects. What are these instruments and how do they compare with analogous living mechanisms in the bodies of bats, porpoises, or whirligig beetles? Footsteps and clickers are simple devices that help blind people create more useful echoes, but the receiving instrument is still the human ear. Perhaps blind men will some day learn to exploit the potentialities of the matchless human brain for a better comprehension of the language of echoes. But, in the meantime, it is important to appre- ciate the devices which men have contrived to carry out both the sending and the receiving functions of echoloca- tion. These mechanisms have been developed for very practical, often military, purposes, excelling particularly in the great distances over which they operate. If they utilize sound waves, they are usually called sonar sys- tems. If electromagnetic waves are employed, they are called radar systems. Sonar is used by man almost ex-

107

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

clusively for underwater echolocation, while radar is used only in air or outer space.

Echoes under Water

The tragic sinking of the Titanic by an iceberg in 1912 prompted the first of many efforts to invent a means of detecting icebergs in darkness or in fog. Even in 1959, icebergs caused the sinking of an ocean liner fully equipped with modem aids to navigation. Sir Hiram Maxim, a prolific inventor who in the late nineteenth century attempted to build flying machines, proposed that bats' methods of navigation be copied directly in the design of safety devices for ocean-going ships. Un- fortunately, however, he did not really know how bats navigate— for the simple reason that the subject had been largely neglected since the days of Spallanzani. He sur- mised correctly that bats used echolocation but was in- correct when he assumed that the probing sound came from the beating of their wings. Hence he advised that ships generate very /ow-frequency sounds of roughly 15 c.p.s. and that receiving devices for such frequencies be mounted on the bow of the ship. Faint echoes from this sound were to ring a small bell, loud ones a large gong, so that the crew could judge the seriousness of the danger.

Maxim's idea was, nevertheless, a step forward in understanding bat navigation, for it introduced for the first time the idea that sounds inaudible to human ears might be the basis of bats' uncanny abihty to fly in dark- ness. But his ideas did not lead to any practical method for detecting icebergs, and for at least two important reasons. In the first place, the low frequencies which he proposed meant that long wave lengths would have been involved; 15-c.p.s. sound has a 20-meter wave. It

108

SONAR AND RADAR

is now well known that objects whose size is much less than the wave length of the sound being used yield only faint echoes, but in 1912 this was not a generally appre- ciated fact. Had scientists been less scornful of bats and had they known more about "Spallanzani's bat prob- lem," more progress would have been made by 1912. Furthermore, Maxim proposed to echolocate icebergs through the air, whereas both the actual danger to the ship and the major part of the iceberg lay beneath the surface. The latter consideration led other inventors to investigate the possibility of using underwater sound.

Two or three years after the sinking of the Titanic, the increasing use of submarines by the German Navy spurred the development of underwater sound devices. At first it was largely a matter of Ustening to the sounds originating from the submarine, particularly from its en- gines and propellers. Much of the naval use of under- water sound is still passive listening for the sounds of other ships. But to a small degree by 1918, and to a much greater extent by 1940, research had led to active probing of the sea with sounds which would yield usable echoes. Enemy submarines were the main military tar- gets, but along with the development of sonar came the echo sounder, or fathometer, a device to measure the depth of the water. In comparison with an enemy sub- marine (or the fish detected by porpoises), the bottom of the ocean would seem to be an easy target, but for many years the idea proved simpler than its realization. In the deeper parts of the ocean even an echo from the bottom was faint and diflBcult to detect with the early sonar devices, but the most critical problem came when the water was shallower and more dangerous. Here the diflSculty was that the ship's hull had a disconcerting tendency to "ring" or prolong the outgoing sound even after the actual generating mechanism had been turned

109

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

off. The combined sound lasted much longer than the time required for it to make the round trip to the bot- tom. In other words, there were severe problems of dis- cnw/wa//(7«— separating relatively faint echoes from the continuing, original emitted sound. The instruments were confronted with the same problems as those that make a blind man less skilled at echolocation than a porpoise or a bat. This engineering problem was solved in part by learning how to make underwater sounds of shorter duration.

By the 1950s, however, echo sounders had been per- fected to a level of reUability where they have become al- most essential for safe navigation. They even became so sensitive that they began to indicate "false bottoms" be- tween the ship and the true bottom. "Finding" two or three extra ocean bottoms above the real one was a rather disconcerting discovery, but after a time the fish- ermen who used echo sounders began to notice that some ' of the "false bottoms" were really echoes from schools of fish. Still later, mysterious layers of faint echoing, or sound scattering, were noted almost everywhere in deep oceans at several hundred feet below the surface. These have been called the deep scattering layers and they were later found to migrate up and down with dawn and dusk. This fact provided the clue to their identity.

Oceanographers had already discovered by systematic netting operations that large populations of shrimp and other small marine animals five at depths where sunlight barely penetrates. This depth is greater at noon than at midnight; hence there is a massive vertical migration of these animals upward during the evening and down again at daybreak. The physical records of the deep scattering layers turned out to match the known be- havior of the animals. Once this additional fact was es- tablished, the echo sounder became a valuable tool for

110

SONAR AND RADAR

biological research, for now the timing of the vertical migrations could be studied with great precision. Of course the echoes from a deep scattering layer do not identify the actual animals, so we still do not know for certain whether the principal sources of these echoes are shrimp-like animals, fish, or possibly squid.

Sonar systems effective at echolocating submarines were used with great success in World War II. One of these sonar systems has a transmitting hydrophone, or underwater loudspeaker which broadcasts sound whose power level is 600 watts. For comparison, the minimum sound power level audible in a quiet room at the fre- quencies to which the human ear is most sensitive is 10-16 ^att, while a very loud shout at close range has a power of 10~^ watt. Thus this sonar system puts into the ocean a sound power roughly equivalent to that of 6,000,000 loud shouts. These intense probing sounds are emitted as short pulses lasting one or two tenths of a second. The frequency can be set anywhere between 10,000 and 26,000 c.p.s. Since the velocity of sound in sea water is about 1500 meters/second, the actual wave lengths of these sounds are from 5 to 13 centimeters, and the length of the pulse is from 150 to 300 meters.

Because this system emits some frequencies above the range of human hearing, there has to be a system to make these frequencies audible. You may be famihar with the "beat note," or "beat frequency" which is con- spicuous when two nearly identical notes are sounded simultaneously. If one note is 500 c.p.s. and the other is 600 c.p.s., you will hear a third note of 100 c.p.s. Hence in the electrical circuit of the sonar apparatus, by generating a local frequency and combining with it the incoming echo, an audible beat note is generated. For instance, an incoming echo of 22,000 c.p.s. and a local frequency of 23,000 c.p.s. produce an audible note

111

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

of 1000 c.p.s. Since the emitted sounds were of short duration, the beat note was also short and sounded Hke "ping." So familiar was this noise to antisubmarine sailors that probing with sound came to be called "pinging."

In selecting the frequencies of the underwater sound which will produce the most useful echoes, the same gen- eral considerations apply as apply to echolocation by bats or bUnd men. Short pulses are desirable because they allow the emitted sound to end before the echo re- turns. This means that the frequency of the waves within the pulse cannot be too low; otherwise the pulse duration allows time for only one or two sound waves. Even sub- marines are small enough targets that long wave lengths could become inefficient owing to the smaller echo re- turned by an object smaller than one wave length. Furthermore, the background noise always present in the sea is greater at lower frequencies. On the other hand, in water as in air, there is an increasing loss of sound energy as the frequency increases because of the absorption of sound as it travels through the water. Bats have evolved a most satisfactory machinery for echolo- cation, but men designing sonar systems had to balance all these factors against one another in reaching the com- promise choice of 10,000-26,000 c.p.s. as a useful range for practical echo ranging.

In view of the fact that many of the most successful bats use signals with a rapid frequency change during each brief pulse of sound, it is interesting to find that sonar engineers developed a somewhat similar procedure which sometimes improves the performance of the sys- tem. In one type of operation the frequency of the emitted sonar signal was varied continuously from 800 c.p.s. above to 800 cycles below the regular frequency. This change was made to occur, as it does in the pulses

112

SONAR AND RADAR

of the FM bats, during each individual pulse of sound. When the echoes of such pulses were received, the frequency change was audible in the beat note. In one typical setting of the apparatus the transmitted fre- quency, and of course the echo, was varied from 20,800 to 19,200 c.p.s. If the local frequency was set at 19,000 c.p.s., the beat note would vary from 1800 to 200 c.p.s. and this would produce an extreme chirp or "Wheeoough" sound. One advantage of this type of op- eration was that at any particular instant of time the many reverberations or multiple echoes from the ship's hull and the water surface had traveled different dis- tances and hence had different frequencies as they ar- rived at the receiving hydrophone. This tended to create an audible difference between the important chirping echoes from a submarine and the noise level of rever- beration from which they had to be discriminated. The important echo was a clear chirp, the competing rever- berations an irregular and shifting mixture of frequen- cies. Very likely bats obtain a similar advantage from their frequency-modulated pulses.

In another type of operation the sonar system used a constant frequency in the emitted pulse, and the opera- tor listened for slight differences in the pitch of the audible beat note. Slight differences between the echo frequency and the local frequency can produce large changes in the audible ping. These differences can be used to determine the relative motion of the target by means of what is called the Doppler effect. This change in frequency resulting from the motion of the source causes the rising pitch of a train whistle as the train ap- proaches you. To understand the Doppler effect, let us consider a concrete example. Suppose that the sonar ship is moving east at 10 meters per second while emit- ting a 0.1 second pulse of 20,000 c.p.s. sound, that is,

113

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

2000 sound waves altogether. Let us simplify our arith- metic by assuming that the velocity of sound in sea water is exactly 1500 meters per second. If the ship were stationary, the pulse would occupy 1500 X 0.1, or 150 meters of distance through the water. But it is moving at 10 meters per second, or 1 meter in the one tenth second required to emit the 2000-wave pulse. Since the transmitting hydrophone pursued the sound waves and covered 1 meter while emitting the pulse, the train of waves was thereby compressed into 149 meters instead of 150. This does not affect the velocity of sound in sea water, so that a passing porpoise would hear the pulse as 2000 waves occupying 149 meters and traveling like all other sound waves at 1500 meters per second. All the waves of the pulse strike the porpoise in 140/1500, or 0.099 second, and their frequency would therefore be 2000 waves in 0.099 second, or 20,202 c.p.s. In other words, the emitted pulse has a higher frequency to the listening porpoise because the ship has moved during the process of emitting it. The velocity of sound depends entirely upon the medium in which it is travel- ing, not upon the velocity of the sound source.

Let us carry our example a little further and suppose that this pulse strikes a submarine which is moving west, toward the sonar ship, also at 10 meters per sec- ond. The pulse, which was 149 meters long as it passed the porpoise, is further compressed during the 0.1 sec- ond while it is coUiding with the oncoming submarine. As it is bouncing back from the target, it is again com- pressed, both times by the same factor of 149/150. It is not necessarily easy to see why this compression oc- curs twice on striking the submarine, but an imaginary modification of the physical events may help. Suppose that the submarine did not return the echo by immediate reflection but rather was equipped with a hydrophone

114

SONAR AND RADAR

and tape recorder so that the pulse was stored on tape. Suppose that at some later time this recording was played back into the water. The compression would oc- cur during both reception and rebroadcast of the sound waves, since in both cases the submarine would be mov- ing relative to the water. Now suppose that the delay between recording and playback is made less and less. Nothing we do while shortening the delay time would affect the compression of the train of sound waves, so that there will still be two such compressions regardless of whether the delay is long or short. If the delay is very short, it approaches zero, and zero delay brings us back to the original situation of immediate reflection. Thus the porpoise hears the echo as 2000 waves occupying only about 147 meters. To be sure, one can spUt hairs and say that 150 X 149/150 X 149/150 X 149/150 are a very little more than 147. But it is not much more, and I promised to keep our arithmetic as simple as possible. Finally the 2000 sound waves reach the receiving hydrophone of the sonar ship, which is still advancing at 10 meters per second to meet them, and the same compression is repeated for the last time. The end re- sult is that the receiving circuit of the sonar system gets the 2000 waves in a shorter time than was required to send them out. The amount of this shortening is

149 0.1-0.1 ("jTfT^*' °^ approximately 0.03 second.

The Doppler effect can be somewhat simplified by considering only the relative motion of the sonar system and its target; in this example the two were approaching at 20 meters per second. The pulse length of the received echo is then reduced by the square of the ratio of the relative velocity of approach to the velocity of sound. It is obvious that if the two ships were moving away from each other, the Doppler effect would work in the

115

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

opposite direction, and the net effect would be a reduc- tion in the frequency of the echo.

To return to our specific example, the final echo has

150 a frequency at the sonar ship of 20,000 X (tt^)*? ot

about 20,540 c.p.s. If this is translated into an audible ping by combining it with a local frequency of 19,000 c.p.s., the echo beat note will be 1540 c.p.s., whereas if both ships were stationary, the beat note would be 1000 c.p.s. This is a fairly extreme example of rapid approach of the two ships, but in actual practice sonar operators can tell when a submarine turns or even when it speeds up or slows down. Though we understand far less of what goes on in a bat or porpoise brain than we know about the operation of this sonar system, it is rea- sonable to infer that similar comparisons of outgoing and echo frequencies may well be used to detect the motion of flying insects or swimming fish. The horse- shoe bats with their constant frequency pulses can per- haps make better use of the Doppler effect than can the FM bats, but even the latter seem to use less frequency sweep when closing on insect prey than during cruising flights when they are presumably seeking to make their initial contact and detection.

Prospecting by Echo

Sound waves are not limited to air and water; they can also travel through solid materials of any kind. Even the echo sounder designed only to echolocate the bot- tom may sometimes show a type of false bottom differ- ent from the fish echoes or deep scattering layer de- scribed earlier. Sometimes the records indicate a second or third bottom below the real one rather than above it. This means that after the bottom echo of the probing

116

SONAR AND RADAR

pulse has returned to the ship's hull a further echo re- turns somewhat later. On first seeing such a record, an experienced physicist might surmise that the pulse had made two round trips through the depth of water under the ship's hull— down to the bottom, up to the surface, down to the bottom again, and finally back as a second echo. This can indeed happen, but then the time of ar- rival of the second echo is almost exactly twice that re- quired by the first. Many of the false bottoms that seem to lie below the real bottom result from echoes return- ing at other times than twice the travel time of the first, direct echo. What really happens under certain condi- tions is that some of the sound energy penetrates into the mud or sand of the ocean floor, travels downward through it, and is then reflected back again by some sudden discontinuity such as a layer of rock of different hardness or density. Making due allowances for the velocity of sound transmission through the material just below the bottom of the ocean, geologists can esti- mate rather accurately the depth below the bottom at which this discontinuity occurs. Without even intending to do so, designers and users of echo sounders have thus hit upon a method of echolocation underground.

Quite purposefully and for many years, other geolo- gists have been studying the transmission of sound waves through miles of the earth's crust. Earthquakes produce vibrations that can be detected by deUcate vibration de- tectors known as seismographs. So do man-made ex- plosions if they are sufficiently violent. Blasting in mines and quarries can be detected miles away, and the seismographic detection of nuclear explosions has now become a matter of major importance, a hotly debated issue at international conferences. By comparing the vibration records resulting from earthquakes at different points around the world, it is possible to deduce that

1 17

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

some waves travel close to the surface, others through deeper layers of rock, while still others travel hundreds of miles below the surface. Careful study of the times of arrival of such waves at different Ustening stations has enabled geologists to learn much more than they could have determined by any other method about the composition of our planet. (The Science Study book How Old Is the Earth goes into this subject in more detail.)

The actual waves recorded by a seismograph are of quite low frequency, and they are usually so irregular that it is difi&cult or even meaningless to describe them in terms of frequencies. Major components vary from about 0.5 to 5 c.p.s. They also differ from sound waves in air or water in that they involve motion in directions other than the direction of wave propagation. There are several different types of seismic waves, classified accord- ing to the relative degrees of motion in various direc- tions. By painstaking analysis of recordings made at various points above and below the ground and in dif- ferent directions from the place of a test explosion, geologists can locate many kinds of rock structures be- low the surface. This procedure has been of great use in prospecting for oil, or rather for the types of rock and salt deposits that are commonly associated with it. Much of our industrial economy has been made possible by the success of this method for echolocating oil.

Echoes versus X-rays

Sound waves have also come into widespread use for harmless testing of materials such as metals and rubber. If the material is pure and homogeneous, it transmits sound waves in a smooth and orderly way. But if there are discontinuities, such as air bubbles in castings or

118

SONAR AND RADAR

defects in tire casings, they distort transmitted sound waves. In some cases very short pulses of sound are used to produce distinct echoes in the material being tested. The sound frequencies are often very high, up to 1 megacycle per second (10^ c.p.s.)? and this is possible because relatively short distances of transmission are in- volved. It is a comparatively inexpensive method of test- ing compared to structural failure of an important and costly machine, and the material is not damaged in any way.

Recently this sort of acoustic probing has been ap- plied to the living bodies of animals and men. It is pos- sible to detect discontinuities in our internal organs in this way, using sound waves generated at the surface of the body by suitable sound sources, such as crystals which are vibrated at high frequencies by electric cur- rents. This method is not without its dangers, for intense sound waves in our bodies can produce damage. But, when properly controlled, the method has some advan- tages over X-rays. At least any damage is local and, inso- far as we know, is not a long-delayed effect on our genes— the complex molecules in our reproductive or- gans, some of which may in time determine what our children will be like. One hmitation of this method stems from the large number of discontinuities that are natu- rally present in a human body— those between muscle and bone, digestive tract and blood vessels, heart and lungs, etc. Thus any abnormaUties must be discriminated from a complex background of natural structures, and this makes it more difficult to locate a tumor in a human brain than an air bubble in a cast-iron pipe. Neverthe- less, this new means for studying our invisible insides may lead in time to safer or more effective methods of locating internal disorders in an early and curable stage. The discrimination problems may be no more difficult

119

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

than those facing a bhnd man or a bat, and human m- genuity may eventually solve this type of problem along with the others mentioned in previous chapters.

Radar

The detection of distant aircraft by echoes of radio waves stands as one of mankind's major technical ac- complishments. In miUtary results alone it has well re- paid the billions of dollars spent on its development and on manufacture of military radar systems. Not only can ground- or ship-based radar systems detect airplanes at hundreds of miles but smaller radars carried on air- planes can locate other aircraft and also resolve a sur- prising amount of detail on the ground below. Radar systems developed for the purpose can draw crude but highly useful maps of hundreds of square miles of ter- rain in a fraction of a second. The maps are drawn on specialized cathode-ray oscilloscope screens. Radar echoes can also be used to locate and track clouds and storms, birds and locusts, meteors, earth satelUtes, and ballistic missiles. Shortly after World War II, radar echoes were successfully detected from the moon. In 1958, for the first time, very faint echoes from the planet Venus were detected. Although this book can- not discuss radar thoroughly, certain basic similarities are well worth considering, and it is even possible to make a rough comparison of the performance and effi- ciencies of radar systems and natural hving systems that have evolved to enable bats to navigate and catch insects in the dark.

Relative Efficiency of Bats and Radar

As with the sonar system we discussed, this compari- son wiU be based on radar systems that served well in

120

SONAR AND RADAR

World War II and have since been retired to pasture- replaced by somewhat more efficient models. To make the comparison more meaningful, I have selected a typical airborne radar set which was a real triumph of engineering skill in that it accompUshed, with a relatively small weight and power consumption, as much as many previous models that were far bulkier and less efficient. This radar operated at a frequency of 9375 megacycles

(X-3x X IQQ), or a wave length of 3.2

y.j /J

centimeters. While this frequency is vastly higher than those used by bats, porpoises, or naval sonar systems, the wave length is not greatly different because of the much higher velocity of light or other electromagnetic radiation. Where our sonar system emitted its acoustic signals at a peak power level of 600 watts, this radar developed a peak power of 10,000 watts. It is important to stress that none of these systems, living or instrumen- tal, emits power continuously; all have a relatively low duty cycle, or ratio of time on to time off. In typical op- eration this radar emitted pulses lasting 0.8 microsecond (8 X 10~^ second) at a pulse repetition rate of 810 pulses per second. In other words, every 1/8 10th second, or 1.23 X 10-3 second it emitted a pulse lasting 8 X 10""^ second, followed by a silent interval about 1500 times as long. This left ample time for echoes to return (at the velocity of light) before the next pulse arrived. The entire radar system weighed 124 pounds, but this does not include the weight of the airplane generator which supplied the electric power. This radar set detected air- craft at 50 miles under most conditions and was a bril- liant operational success. It is therefore of some interest to inquire how well it compares with bat systems, watt for watt of power emitted and gram for gram of weight. This comparison is not a simple one because of the

121

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

widely different circumstances in which the two classes of echo-ranging systems are actually used. Bats are in- terested in detecting small insects at a few feet or yards. The user of an airborne radar wishes to locate objects on the ground and other airplanes some miles away. Bats use sound waves, while radar employs radio waves of only sUghtly greater wave lengths. Bats maneuver very rapidly, the whole sequence of detection, turning toward an insect, intercepting, catching, and swallowing, all oc- curring withm 1 second. In ordinary use of an airborne radar, the operator sees a spot on his oscilloscope screen, notes how it changes in position relative to his own flight path, and then takes appropriate action. This may vary all the way from a turn to avoid any danger of collision, if the two airplanes are airhners, to a close pursuit and firing of a machine gun or rocket at the other plane if it is an enemy in time of war. In both cases the whole operation may be accompUshed by a man sitting in a darkened cabin looking only at spots on his radar screen. The bat does it all within one sec- ond, in the dark, with a brain smaller than the eraser on a pencil.

To make comparison a quantitative one, we can best tabulate the important quantities which are known for the two systems and on which we may base estimates of their relative efficiencies. The table on page 123 gives approximately the range of the radar and also its weight and power requirements. An efficient system for echo- location should detect the smallest possible objects at the greatest possible distances and it should do so with the least possible power and the lightest possible appa- ratus. Bulky installations of whirling machinery may be impressive at first glance, but unnecessary complexity and power expenditure are actually signs of inefficiency. With this in mind, let us set up an efficiency index, an

122

SONAR AND RADAR

equation which will evaluate the combination of these four important factors. Such an index should have a high value for the most efficient systems and should be roughly proportional to the relative efficiencies of the various systems of echolocation that we compare. As will become clear a Uttle later, this is not as simple as it might seem, but the process of attempting to define such an index, and then modifying it as may seem nec- essary, will in itself prove to be helpful in calling atten- tion to the various quantitative considerations that are important for echolocation.

TABULAR COMPARISONS OF BATS AND RADAR

AN/APS - 10

radar system

Big

brown bat

Target detected Target diameter, d

(cm) Range of detection, R

(cm) Weight of apparatus, W

(grams) Emitted power, P

(watts) R/PWd RVPWd^ RVPWd^

Airplane Insect

300

8X10^

9X10*

10*

2.9 X 10-^

5 X 10^3

5.5 X 108

200 0.1

Little

brown

bat

Wire

1.8 X 10-2

90

0.05

10-5 10-6

2 X 108 iQii

1.6X10^5 3.8X10^^ 1.6X1015 1.2X1022

The above table Usts the range of detection, R, the diameter of the target, d, (both in centimeters), the power emission, P (watts) , and the weight of the system, W (grams). For the bats, 10 per cent of the weight of a fasting animal seems a generous allowance for the lar-

123

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

ynx, ears, auditory portions of the brain, and the other parts used directly for echolocation. For both bat and radar the power is the peak level reached during each pulse. It may be recalled from Chapter 2 that the ears of bats and men operate at sound power levels ranging from about 10"^^ to 10"* watt per square centimeter. The air- borne radar detection of another airplane at 50 miles is compared with two cases of echolocation by bats— the detection of a 1 -centimeter insect (or pebble) by a big brown bat at 2 meters, and the echolocation of a 0.18- millimeter wire by a Uttle brown bat at 90 centimeters. The j5rst approach to defining the efficiency index might be simply to have R, the distance of detection, in the numerator, and the other three quantities, P (power), W (weight), and d (target size), in the de- nominator, where large values will tend to lower the in- dex. This index, R/PWd, is listed in the next row of the table, and when judged on this basis, the bats appear bil- lions of times better than the radar system. But a little reflection shows that, in defining the index in this way, we have made an important assumption; namely, that these four quantities are really related to one another as we have entered them in the equation. For example, this definition of efficiency assumes that range will in- crease in direct proportion to power. But for all radar systems, and probably all bats, the emitted energy falls off as the square of the distance. And most small targets send back echoes that also obey the inverse-square law. As pointed out in Chapter 4, where insect-catching bats were compared with the hypothetical case of a bat at- tempting to echolocate fish through the air-water inter- face, the energy in an echo is proportional to 1/R*. This means that to obtain twice the range a system of echolocation will need 2*, or 16 times as much power, and we should therefore change our index to contain R

124

SONAR AND RADAR

to the fourth power instead of the first. This will greatly increase the rating scored by the radar set detecting an airplane at 50 miles.

Having made this improvement in the index, we should also scrutinize the other variables in our equa- tion, in particular the size of the target, d. If a series of targets is fairly large relative to the wave length of the signal being used to generate an echo, the echo power is usually proportional roughly to their areas, or to d^. This is true of most radar targets, and certainly of air- planes being echolocated with 3.2-centimeter waves. Is it also true for bats? The insects they catch vary from somewhat below one wave length to several wave lengths, and of course the FM bats employ orientation sounds containing a whole octave of frequencies, or a twofold range of wave lengths in each pulse. It is prob- ably reasonable to assume that in insect detection the echo power varies as the square of the target diameter, although in some cases the insects may be enough below one wave length so that this assumption would lead to an overestimate of the echo strength. The next line of the table therefore lists for each of the three systems the value of the revised efi&ciency index, R^/PWd^. Even on this basis the bats are somewhat superior to the radar.

Finally, we should pay a little more attention to the bats which detect wires far smaller than one wave length, such as the little brown bat listed in the third row of the table. When wires or other cylindrical obstacles are much smaller than one wave length, the echo power var- ies as d^, and the 0.1 8 -millimeter wires detected at 90 centimeters are certainly in this size range. This domain of target size produces what is sometimes called Rayleigh scattering, after the nineteenth-century physicist who analyzed it with special reference to Ught scattered by tiny particles in the air. Such light makes up most of

125

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

what we see in the sky, and since the particles are of less than the wave lengths of visible light (4 to 7 X 10"^ centimeter), short wave length light is more strongly scattered than other colors. This is why the sky is blue. By analogy we might say that the bat flying up to these wires must hear "blue echoes." In any event, a case could be made for evaluating bat sonar by means of an index containing d* rather than d or d-, and the value of R^/PWd^ is therefore listed in the last Une of the table. The drastic results of changing the definition of our efficiency index should now be clear. This may indeed open serious questions as to whether such different sys- tems for echolocation can be meaningfully compared on a simple numerical basis. Furthermore, several other im- portant factors have not yet been brought into the com- parison. Bats operating with sound waves in air face serious reductions in echo signal due to the absorption of sound in air, especially at higher frequencies. During the round trip from bat to target and return, sound of 50 kc loses power by a factor of 0.63 for every meter of distance, in addition to the reduction due to the in- verse fourth power reduction for echoes. At 100 kc the reduction is by a factor of 0.44 over every meter. Radio waves suffer no such severe losses in traveling through the air. This fact puts the bat at a great disadvantage over long distances. On the other hand, there is a con- sideration which would favor most radar systems as compared to bats. This is the duty cycle, or fraction of the time during which energy is being emitted. In typical cases, such as those included in our table, a bat would be emittmg 10 to 20 pulses per second, each pulse lasting 2 to 5 milliseconds, so that the duty cycle would vary between 0.02 and 0.1. The radar had a far lower duty cycle, however, the interval between pulses having been about 1500 times as long as the pulse itself, so that the

126

SONAR AND RADAR

duty cycle would be about 0.0007. This means that if we were to use average power rather than peak power in our comparison, the bats would suffer by a factor of about 100. Yet a partisan of the bats might offer in rebuttal the consideration that we allowed 10 per cent of the animal's weight for its sonar apparatus, whereas the weight of the radar set was a much smaller fraction of the mass of the airplane that carried it. From the bat's point of view it would perhaps be more valid to compare its whole weight with that of the entire airplane.

If we take the broadest view, it is obvious that bats and other Uving animals are vastly more efficient than radars and airplanes, even though it is difficult to attach numbers to the comparison. Bats maintain and repair their Uving machinery; airplanes and radar sets must be manufactured and repaired by men. Bats catch and di- gest all the food that provides power for their bodily mechanisms; airplanes are not expected to refuel by catching birds, and the fuel pumped to them requires no chemical processing in the plane before use. Nor do any artifical mechanisms reproduce themselves. The unusual aspect of the comparison we have been making is that a Hving mechanism can be compared directly with a radar set on almost the same terms that an engineer would employ in comparing one radar with another. The re- sults of the comparison inspire a healthy respect for the mechanisms of flesh and blood which have evolved in nature under the pressure of natural selection.

127

CHAPTER 6 Suppose You Were Blind

In the preceding chapters we have examined waves and echoes to understand better how animals and men have used them to locate objects which are essential for sur- vival. Such studies of natural phenomena often seem use- less to all but a very few people, but so do many scientific explorations. Yet history has clearly shown that men have improved their lot by investigations into the un- known. However insignificant it may have seemed at the time, there is a true inner satisfaction in discovering new relationships and new information to add to our under- standing of the world around us. We often hope that observations and new facts can one day be used to im- prove our environment still further. What could be more beneficial than trying to apply this new-found knowledge to men who cannot see with their eyes? Can we help them to "see" with their ears— to learn the language of echoes?

Blindness is always a tragedy for human beings be- cause our brains and our whole way of life are built around light and vision. But men's eyes are not their only channels of communication with the rest of the world,

129

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

and sound is in some ways even more useful. For exam- ple, we can see somewhat less than 1 octave of frequen- cies, or wave lengths, roughly from 4 to 7.5 X 10""^ centi- meter. Our sense of hearing, on the other hand, extends from about 20 to 20,000 c.p.s., a range of a thousand- fold, or approximately 10 octaves. Audible sound can thus contain a much richer variety of frequencies than visible light, and this is partly why sound rather than Ught is used for speech. Of course, there are other rea- sons; for instance, living organisms cannot generate hght, except for a few luminescent animals and plants.

The sharp shadows cast by light make it less useful as a vehicle for speech and short-range communication. Just because sound does go around comers, it is useful in calling and signaling, particularly when almost every motion and contact between a person or animal and the physical world around it generates some sound. The great advantage of light to us is that it has short wave lengths and, consequently, objects of small size give off specular reflections. It is for this reason that eyes and lenses can focus sharp images. Only when one tries to use a microscope to see objects about the size of the wave length of light does that wave length become an important Hmitation. An object must be smaller than one micron (one millionth of a meter) before it scatters light rather than reflecting it.

If sound waves and light waves did not already exist, we well might find scientists trying to invent them, one to form sharp images and permit accurate observations of small details, the other with a wide-frequency spec- trum to convey complex information with a minimum of interference from shadow-casting obstacles. The two types complement each other, and while the loss of our sense organs for either is a major handicap, there is

130

SUPPOSE YOU WERE BLIND

enough duplication of what each can do to permit some substitution of one after losing the other.

The Sense of Obstacles

Blindness has been an all-too-common aflSiction of men, and while no device or procedure can completely replace lost sight, blind men for centuries have learned to get about in the world and carry on a surprising num- ber of activities. Some become so skillful at avoiding ob- stacles and maintaining an adequate general orientation that it is difficult for a stranger to realize they are really blind. For example, there was once a blind boy who learned when six years old to ride his tricycle all about the sidewalks near his home without injury or accident. When he approached pedestrians, he steered around them, and he always knew when to turn corners without going into the street. Other blmd people travel widely in busy cities, crossing streets, using buses and trains, dodging lampposts and wire fences. How do they detect these obstacles before touching them? Many theories have been advanced, both by the blind people themselves and by those who have worked or lived with them. Cu- riously enough, the most skillful of the bUnd differ widely in their explanations of their own abilities. Many say they feel with then* hands or faces the proximity of obstacles, and the term "facial vision" has come into wide use to describe their orientation to objects which are too far away to feel or touch. Others believe that hearing is somehow involved; still others speak of "pres- sures" and other ill-defined sensations that warn them of dangers just ahead.

The central question is obviously the nature of the physical message that travels from the obstacles to the bhnd man, and the way in which his remaining sense

131

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

organs detect and interpret this information from the outside world. From about 1890 to 1940 many studies were made of the "sense of obstacles," but only in the early 1940s was a conclusive answer obtained from care- fully controlled experiments. While these experiments were performed by men who called themselves psycholo- gists, the experiments can be considered classic examples of biophysics, the application to problems posed by liv- ing organisms of the same basic principles of investiga- tion that have developed physics as a rigorous science. The chief difference between biophysics, thus broadly defined, and the physics of non-living systems is the greater degree of complexity and refinement of living organisms. Animals and men are made of far more intricate mechanisms than clickers and ripple tanks, mi- croscopes or television sets, and this is why our under- standing of biological processes is so much less thor- ough and complete than our knowledge of physics or chemistry.

The psychologists, or biophysicists, who finally solved the question of obstacle perception by the blind were Professor Karl M. Dallenbach of Cornell University, and two graduate students, one of whom, Michael Supa, was himself totally blind. Milton Cotzin, the other student, had normal vision, but he and others who served as ex- perimental subjects wore blindfolds for many hours at a time in order to experience what life is Hke for the blind, and, in particular, to develop as much as possible the ability to detect obstacles before bumping into them. First the experimenters set up a sort of obstacle course, a long hallway down which the subject walked and across which was placed a large screen of fiberboard at some point chosen by the experimenter. This location was var- ied from trial to trial, so that the subject never knew whether it was 6, 10, 18, 24, or 30 feet ahead of the

132

SUPPOSE YOU WERE BLIND

Starting point, or even whether it was there at all. His task was to walk along the hallway, say when he first thought he was approaching the screen, and then walk up as close as he could without striking it.

Some of the subjects, both blind and blindfolded, could judge accurately the presence or absence of the screen at several feet and then move in untU their faces were within a few inches before deciding that any further approach would bring them into contact with it. The phenomenon of obstacle detection was thus brought into the laboratory in a manner which allowed it to be studied repeatedly under reasonably constant conditions. This step is often a crucial one in attacking scientific problems of this sort. Elusive and unpredictable events are very much more difl&cult to study than those which can be repeated under known conditions. Only in the latter case is it fairly easy to vary the factors that seem likely to be important and then observe the results. EarUer studies of obstacle detection by the bUnd had been plagued with great variability in the performance of the subjects. That mainly is why they had not led to clear and decisive answers. Yet Supa, Cotzin, and Dallenbach built their experimental design on the extensive, if inconclusive, experience of earUer experimenters. Without this back- ground they would probably not have been able to devise such decisive experiments.

Once they had arranged conditions where blind or bUndfolded people were regularly detecting a standard- ized test obstacle, the next step was the theoretically ob- vious but nevertheless rather difficult one of eUminating one possible channel of sensory communication at a time, while leaving the subject with free use of the others. One leading theory was that the skin supplied some kind of sensation of touch or pressure when obstacles were nearby; another was that sound played a major role. The

133

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

practical problem in testing the "skin pressure theory" was to shield the subjects' skin from any possible in- fluence that might be arriving from the obstacle, and this was doubly difi&cult because no one could say what this might be— air currents, electromagnetic radiation, heat or cold, or possibly some sort of energy not known to physics. To test the sound theory, the logical proce- dure was to prevent sounds from reaching the subjects' ears without interfering with whatever the skin might be feeling as a result of proximity to the obstacles. The covering of the skin clearly had to be accomplished with- out interfering with the subjects' hearing, and vice versa. The final outfit that the subjects were obliged to wear consisted of a long veil of thick felt which covered the head and shoulders, plus heavy leather gloves to shield the hands. Ordinary clothing covered the rest of the body surface. Such was the protection that they could not feel even the air current of an electric fan directed at their heads. After some preliminary trials to accustom them to walking about in this "armor," the subjects found they could detect the screen almost as well as ever. The average distance of first detection had been 6.9 feet with no veil or gloves, and it was now reduced only slightly —to 5.25 feet. This seemed to dispose of the possibility that obstacles were detected by feehng them through the skin, despite the fact that originally some of this group of subjects, like many blind people, were certain that they felt the obstacles with the hands or face.

The next experiment was to leave the hands and face completely free but to cover the subjects' ears. Earlier experiments of this kind had given conflicting results; sometimes the detection of obstacles was impaired, sometimes not. Complete exclusion of sound by ear- plugs is not possible, but Supa, Cotzin, and Dallenbach wished to be sure that as httle sound as possible reached

134

SUPPOSE YOU WERE BLIND

their subjects. They therefore wore earplugs of wax and cotton, ear muffs, and padding over the sides of the head. This compound series of barriers was necessary because many sounds, particularly those of low frequency, pene- trate ordinary earplugs or ear muffs. Everyone knows from the ordinary experience of wearing ear muffs or parka hoods in cold weather that by speaking slightly louder than usual one can still converse with his com- panions no matter how well the ears are protected from the winter winds.

So thorough was this muffling that the subjects could not hear the sounds of their own footsteps, and instruc- tions could only be given them by loud shouts. A loud shout can easily have 10^ times the energy of a barely audible whisper. Direct measurement of the intensity necessary for them to detect a test sound showed that their auditory sensitivity had been reduced by a factor of about 4,000,000; that is, they could hear the test sound only after its energy level had been increased four millionfold above the level that was just audible without the ear covering.

When the same subjects were now asked to repeat the experiments with their hearing thus impaired, the results were spectacular. None retained any obstacle -detection ability at all, and in each of one hundred trials every subject bumped unexpectedly into the screen. One of the blind men, who had stoutly maintained that sound played no part at all in his "facial vision," complained that he was now getting no sensation at all, and for the first time he walked hesitantly and held out his hands to guard against anticipated accidents. If sound does ac- count for the obstacle-detection ability, one might ask why there was any reduction in distance of first detection when the subjects wore the felt veil and leather gloves.

135

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

This was probably due to the reduction in sound level caused by the shielding effect of the bulky hood.

Guiding Echoes

These experiments would seem to have settled the matter once and for all, but criticisms would still have been possible if the experimenters had stopped at this point. Perhaps the pressure of the ear covering was dis- turbing some subtle tactile sense. Perhaps blind men were warned of obstacles not by hearing as such but by some special kind of pressure sense involving the ear canal or adjacent areas of skin. Even men who had stud- ied this subject for years were skeptical that sound waves could be the messengers by which blind people detected obstacles. Further, many blind men themselves still con- tinued to think they felt obstacles. To convince such skeptics it was necessary to modify the experiment so that sound and only sound carried the necessary infor- mation from the outside world into the subject's nervous system. This might seem a hopeless task; if the experi- ments described above were unconvincing, what argu- ments could hope to overcome such skepticism?

The answer was to employ a telephone system to transmit the appropriate sounds to the subject sitting in a remote and soundproof room. The sounds transmitted over the telephone wires were those picked up by a mi- crophone carried by a man walking along the same obstacle course. They were similar, though not identical, to what the man would hear himself if he were listening for evidence that the screen was just ahead.

The results of the telephone experiment were aston- ishingly close to those obtained by the same subjects in the original tests. They could sit in the soundproof room and decide by listening to the telephone whether the

136

SUPPOSE YOU WERE BLIND

screen was being approached or not. After some practice they could detect the screen at an average distance of 6.4 feet, only a little less than their average of 6.9 feet when they were doing their own walking and listening. Such a result would seem to dispel all doubts; surely no one could argue now that anything but sound was in- volved. But scientists who have studied problems like this have learned to be extremely cautious. Many experi- ments which have seemed this convincing have turned out to be misleading. Suppose, for example, that the per- son who walked up to the screen with the microphone changed his breathing rhythm or the sounds of his foot- steps and thus unconsciously conveyed to the remote hstener his proxunity to the screen? This sort of uncon- scious signaling has been known to occur, and, inciden- tally, it accounts for many cases of what has been interpreted as mental telepathy.

This worry led to further experiments in which the second person was replaced by a motor-driven cart which carried the microphone towards the screen. The subject in the soundproof room controlled the move- ments of the cart while Ustening to the sounds the mi- crophone picked up. As often happens in a scientific experiment, new facts raise new questions— one often ends up with more questions than he had at the begin- ning. Here the question raised was of major importance. Granted that sounds could be conveyed over the tele- phone system, what were the actual sounds that told the listener the screen was near? In the original experi- ment no special effort was made to generate sounds or produce echoes; indeed, the experimenters in the begin- ning had been uncertain that sounds were really of any consequence. They had simply tried to bring phenome- non into the laboratory and arrange conditions under which it could be repeatedly studied. But having learned

137

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

that sound, rather than anything which could not travel along telephone wires, informed the blind man that the screen was in front of him, the experimenters had to con- sider the nature of these sounds.

Footsteps were an obvious possibility, and when the original experiments were repeated with the subjects walking in their stocking feet on a soft carpet, their ability to detect the screen was greatly reduced. The av- erage distance of first detection fell from 6.9 feet when the subjects were wearing shoes and walking on the bare floor to 3.4 feet when the sounds of their footsteps were dampened by the carpet. Some subjects snapped their fingers or made clucking sounds, but others apparently relied on whatever sounds were present in the hallway, such as the sound of their own breathing or the rustle of their clothing. This question had not been seriously con- sidered in the design of the first experiments, but now that the investigation had reached the point where the microphone was to be mounted on a cart there would be no sound from footsteps or breathing. Some other sound had to be substituted, which provided the opportunity to study the usefulness of various sounds in providing audi- ble clues to the presence or absence of obstacles. Ob- viously, too, the experiment involved echoes. If some sound told a listener that the screen was present, it must have been a sound which was different with the screen than without it.

In order to study the character of the echoes used by blind people, the experimenters then equipped the cart with a loudspeaker as well as a microphone. A variety of sounds with known characteristics could now be gen- erated by the loudspeaker for further tests. If a loud hissing noise was used— that is, a noise containing a wide range of audible frequencies— the screen could be de- tected by the subjects hstening to the telephone in the

138

SUPPOSE YOU WERE BLIND

soundproof room. The distance of first detection aver- aged 3.75 feet, less than the range of detection when in an earlier test a person carried the microphone toward the screen. Nevertheless, it was an impressive perform- ance, considering how greatly the situation had been al- tered from the first series of experiments. Other sounds were also tried, but the experim.ents were concluded be- fore the ideal sound had been discovered which men might use to obtain the more revealing echoes. The in- vestigations ended because the original problem had been conclusively solved by the proof that sounds and, in particular, echoes were the messages that inform blind men about the existence and position of obstacles.

One significant feature of this important discovery is the striking divergence between the subjective feelings of many blind people and all the objective evidence which we have examined. When a man has developed the re- markable abihty to find his way about through the bus- tling traffic of a modern city in what to him is total darkness, and when he does this so skillfully and un- obtrusively that one can travel with him for hours and never suspect that he is blind, then it is natural to assume that he knows what he is doing and how he does it. But often the expert blind man can say only that he some- how "feels" his way and "knows" before he bumps into the tree or fence post that it is there. If questioned more closely, he may say he feels the proximity of the object with his hands, his face, or his forehead. Yet when the process of obstacle detection is studied under controlled conditions, it is clear that sounds and hearing are the essential ingredients. In addition, the whole surface of the blind man's skin can be covered by heavy felt or leather without preventing him. from detecting obstacles before he strikes them. When his ears are plugged, he no longer "feels" the obstacles with his hands or face,

139

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

and if he continues on his way, he invariably strikes them without warning. Subjective impressions obviously can be misleading-we do not always know just which of our senses is informing us about our surroundmgs. This is not to say that our senses are not keen, but rather that our conscious thinking about them may lead us to the wrong conclusion about how they operate.

This is not a unique misapprehension concerning the workings of our sense organs, although perhaps it is an extreme one. Another example also involves the sense of hearing. How do we know where a sound is coming from? Sometimes we see the source and are thus in- formed of its position, but everyone is able to locate the origin of an unfamiliar sound heard in darkness, and usually with great accuracy. Sometimes we locate a sound source approximately by turning our heads until the sound is louder in one ear than in the other, but more often and with great precision we rely on the difference in the same sound as it arrives at the two ears. Consider for the present only one type of sound, a sharp click. The most important property of the bundle of sound waves constituting the click is the time of arrival of the first sound waves at the two ears. If the click comes from straight ahead, the two ears receive the first sound waves at exactly the same time because they are equidistant from the source. If, however, the click arises at some point to the right of the direction you are facmg, it reaches the right ear a small fraction of a second sooner than the left. If the source is 90" to one side, the opposite ear is about 20 centimeters farther away than the closer one, and since sound waves in air travel about 30 centi- meters per millisecond, this means that the maximum possible difference in time of arrival at the two ears is less than 1 millisecond. Yet such is the precision of the auditory portions of our brains that we can easily dis-

140

SUPPOSE YOU WERE BLIND

tinguish between a sound source that is straight ahead and one that is displaced only 10° to one side. If the source is 3 meters away, the 10° displacement which is clearly noticeable involves a difference in time of arrival at our two ears of about 0.1 millisecond. It is difficult

Fig. 15. Your ability to discriminate minute differ- ences in the time of arrival of two sounds can be tested with this device. Any source of sharp clicks will do if it is tightly enclosed in the box so that you hear it only through the tubes.

to locate sound sources accurately if they he directly in front of us, or anywhere in the plane that is equidistant from the two ears. If we have to attempt this, we usually do so by moving our heads about and bringing one ear closer to the source.

The role of differences in time of arrival of a click at

141

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

the two ears can be studied with the aid of a simple device illustrated in Fig. 15. This consists of a source of clicks, which could be a loudspeaker or a mechanical clicker, and a sound-tight box to house it. From the box lead two tubes each ending like a physician's stethoscope, but make sure the earplugs are soft to avoid accidental injury to your ears. One tube is fixed in length, while the other has a telescoping tube like that of a trombone so that its length can be varied. When the two tubes have different lengths, it will obviously require longer for the first sound waves of the click to reach one ear than the other. Since the velocity of sound is known, the differ- ence in time of arrival of the cUcks can be calculated easily from the difference in length of the two tubes. When one listens to clicks through these tubes of un- equal length, the effect is strikingly Uke that of a cHck coming from one side. If the eyes are closed and one makes even a small effort to imagine that the clicks are coming through the open air rather than through the tubes, there is a compeUing illusion that the source is at the side of the ear receiving the shorter tube. Of course it makes no difference where the box containing the source of cHcks is really situated, nor does the actual length of the two tubes matter. When precise measure- ments are made with more refined apparatus of this same type, the minimum time difference that leads to this illu- sion of a source at one side or the other is less than 0.1 millisecond.

We need experiments like these to tell us about one of the principal ways in which we locate the source of a sound. We never think, "That click reached my right ear 1/10,000 second before it got to my left ear; therefore, it must have come from a little to one side of the median plane of my head." We simply recognize that the click came from one side without any idea how we located

142

SUPPOSE YOU WERE BLIND

it. In much the same way, a bUnd man learns to antici- pate colHsions with obstacles under certain conditions, usually without realizing at all that these conditions are the presence of audible echoes. Recognizing the prox- imity of an obstacle and knowing from experience the pain of bumping into it, he comes to believe that he felt its nearness with his hands or face. All this adds up to a warning not to interpret the workings of our sense organs and our brains too hastily; they may be operating in other ways than we are first incHned to think. But we should not go to the other extreme and conclude that measuring instruments will always improve upon our un- aided senses. As we have learned, sense organs and brains of men, porpoises, bats, and beetles accomplish extremely difficult feats of detection and discrimination. To return to the blind man's problems of orientation, echolocation is certainly the technique by which skillful blind people find their way in the "dark." But with the general question thus answered, we are immediately im- pelled to ask what type of sound will provide a blind m.an with the most informative echoes. In the final ex- periments by Supa, Cotzin, and Dallenbach where the cart carried the loudspeaker and microphone up to the test obstacle, it turned out that a hissing noise was more effective than pure tones. But the average distance of detection was only 3.75 feet instead of 6.9. Does this mean that footsteps are more efficient sounds for this purpose, or does it mean that the cart with the loud- speaker and microphone was less easily controlled by the remote listener? From the experiments described in Chapters 2 and 3 it is clear that some sounds generate more useful echoes than others, and that a very short click has the advantage that it ends before the first echo begins to return. But footsteps on the floor are not es- pecially sharp clicks, even when the walker's shoes have

143

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

hard soles. Some blind men prefer shoes with metal heel plates, perhaps because of the sharper footsteps that re- sult. If you have carried out some experiments with clickers, like the one illustrated in Fig. 7, it must be obvious that if you, a rank beginner, can detect trees, an experienced blind man can do at least as well. Many blind men have used clickers of one sort or another— aside from metal heel plates, the canes used by many blind men to lengthen the reach of their hands are used as cUckers by tapping the ground or pavement. The re- sulting cUcks provide a standard noise which gives a useful echo.

But we can properly ask whether footsteps, cane taps, or even toy cUckers mounted in horns are really the best types of sound for a blind man's purposes. Do they gen- erate the most informative possible echoes or are there other types of sound that would be superior? The ques- tion is simply asked, but the search for a convincing answer has been difficult and frustrating. Various types of cHckers and portable sound sources have been built and tested. Some, particularly the directional clickers, have been used extensively by a small number of blind men, including their inventors. But the results have been far from satisfactory, and many users find it too difficult to hear consistent echoes or find the added facility at orientation not worth the embarrassment caused by a conspicuous audible sound that calls attention to their handicap. Yet almost every object that a blind man needs to detect does interact in some way with audible sound waves. This being so, why can we not devise a probing sound which will produce audible echoes that are recog- nizably related to the objects a blind man needs to locate?

One difficulty has already been called to our attention in the experiment where tape recordings of clicks or other impulsive sounds were played backward on a tape

144

SUPPOSE YOU WERE BLIND

recorder. This experiment demonstrated the effectiveness of our built-in suppressor mechanism which renders echoes far less audible because our ears are temporarily insensitive immediately after a loud outgoing sound. A multitude of echoes are clearly audible on reversed play- back when they precede a sharp chck or pistol shot, but are quite unnoticed when they follow the louder sound as they do in ordinary life. Is this the major reason why blind men fail to learn as much from echoes as they theoretically should? And if so, could not some device be developed to overcome this difficulty? No one knows the answers to these questions, and they are good ex- amples of the truism that no branch of science is com- plete or finished. Perhaps some reader may have the ideas and the opportunity to make further advances to- ward a real solution of the bUnd man's problems of orientation. Just because some men have failed so far to find such a solution, others should not be discouraged from new attempts, especially when the potential gains to human welfare are so great.

145

FURTHER READING

Barnes, H. : Oceanography and Marine Biology. Lon- don: George Allen and Unwin, Ltd., 1959.

This up-to-date elementary survey of oceanography includes a chapter on the sounds of marine animals and the use of sound for exploration of the ocean depths.

Boys, C, v.: Soap Bubbles and the Forces Which Mould Them. New York: Science Study Series, Doubleday Anchor Books, 1959.

A small readable classic of science which will give you, among other things, a better understanding of surface tension.

Bowen, E. G. (Editor) : A Textbook of Radar. Cam- bridge, England: Cambridge University Press, 2nd Edi- tion, 1954.

This textbook contains more general background ma- terial, including such fascinating subjects as radar echoes from the moon and the use of radar in navigation.

Buddenbrock, W. von: The Senses. Ann Arbor, Michi- gan: University of Michigan Press, 1958.

A readable and authoritative account of sense organs of all sorts of animals, from the eyes of scallops to inner ears of men.

147

FURTHER READING

Carthy, J. D.: Animal Navigation. London: George

Allen and Unwin, Ltd., 1956.

This popular and readable book describes the orienta- tion and navigation of insects, fishes, birds, and whales as well as those of bats and domestic animals.

Fletcher, H.: Speech and Hearing in Communication. New York: Van Nostrand, 1953.

This thorough and somewhat technical book sum- marizes the extensive researches carried out at the Bell Telephone Laboratories and elsewhere on the physical properties of speech, the mechanisms of human hear- ing, and the nature of hearing losses and deafness.

GriflSn, D. R.: Listening in the Dark. New Haven, Con- necticut: Yale University Press, 1958.

Many aspects of the natural sonar of bats, birds, and porpoises are discussed more fully than in this short monograph, including the many different types of bats and their orientation sounds, their pursuit and capture of flying insects, fish, and other food. There are chapters on echolocation by bUnd men, and on the acuity of echolocation achieved by bats, including their ability to hear faint echoes despite the presence of louder jamming noises.

GriJBBn, D. R.: "Bird Sonar." Scientific American Maga- zine, March 1954. "More about Bat 'Radar.' " Scientific American Mag- azine, August 1958. These articles contain excellent illustrations and sup- plement the chapters of this monograph dealing with the natural sonar of bats and birds.

Horton, J. W.: Fundamentals of Sonar. Annapolis, Maryland: U. S. Naval Institute, 1957.

148

FURTHER READING

This rather technical book describes the basic prin- ciples and operation of electronic sonar systems as they are used on ships. It explains the Doppler effect and other basic phenomena of echolocation with spe- cial reference to underwater sound.

Hurley, P. M.: How Old is the Earth. New York: Science Study Series, Doubleday Anchor Books, 1959.

This modem book on radioactivity as an energy source in the earth and as means of measuring time also includes a piece on seismic waves.

Pierce, J. R., and David, E. E., Jr.: Man's World of Sound. New York: Doubleday and Company, 1958. A semi-popular book describing at an elementary level the physical properties of sound waves. It dis- cusses such matters as interference, the propagation of sound, standing waves, etc., in a way which any student of secondary school physics should have litde diflBculty in understanding.

Ridenour, L. N. (Editor) : Radar System Engineering, New York: McGraw-Hill, 1st Edition, 1947.

This is a general description of the radar systems de- veloped during World War II by the Radiation Labo- ratory at M.I.T. While parts of it are quite technical, many chapters can easily be understood by any seri- ously interested reader of this monograph. Radar sets and systems are described and illustrated in sufi&cient detail to permit, for example, the sort of comparison with biological systems that were discussed in Chap- ter 5.

Rummell, J. A.: "Modern Sonar Systems." Electronics (Engineering Edition), January 1958, pages 58-62. A brief survey of the apparatus used in sonar systems.

149

FURTHER READING

Witcher, C. M., and Washington, L.: "Echo-Location for the Blind." Electronics, December 1954, pages 136-137. A brief but complete description of one of the more successful sound-generating devices used by blind people to find their way about. The late C. M. Witcher was himself blind and he devoted his engineering tal- ents to improving such devices for his own use and for the benefit of other bUnd people.

Zahl, P. A. (Editor) : Blindness. Princeton, New Jersey: Princeton University Press, 1950.

A collection of articles by different authors discussing the most important problems faced by bUnd peo- ple, education for the blind, vocational rehabilitation, talking books, guide dogs, guidance devices, and the remote possibility of direct stimulation of the optic nerves or visual areas of the brain.

150

INDEX

Acoustic energy, 46; in water, 19

Acoustic orientation, 32. See also Echolocation and Orientation

Acoustic probing, 116-20

Acoustics: architectural, 50; echoes, 57-73

Aerial interception, 18

Air, 19, 40, 44, 97, 116; molecules of, 39; sound in, 25

Airplanes, 17, 120

Amazon River, 21

Amplitude, 36

Angle of incidence, 77

Angle of reflection, 77

Animal Navigation, 148

Animals: domestic, 148; echoes, use of, 10; lu- minescent, 19; nocturnal vision of, 27, 32; orientation in darkness, 27-28; see also Bats

Antennae, 54

Apparatus. See Equipment

Aquaria, 26

Architectural acoustics, 50

Auditory nerve, 20

Barnes, H., 147

Bats, 2, 18, 26-32, 44, 121-22, 127, 148; acoustic orienta- tion, 32; brown bats, 85, 88, 93; chirping bats, 87; chirps, 87; ears, 28; echo- location, 29, 78, 90-95, 99- 100, 112; fish-catching, 95-99; flight, 89-90; FM bats, 87, 95, 97, 113, 116, 125; foods, 28, amount of, 88-89; horseshoe bats, 84-85, 87, 116; impair-

ments: of mouth, 29, of sense organs, 28-29; jam- ming, 103; nature of, 26-27, 31; navigation, 2, 27, 32, 108; objects, small, 104; obstacles, 31, 93-95, 100-1, discriminating ability, 103- 4; orientation, 32, 51-53; orientation sounds, 29-31, 84-87; rhythm, 89-90; sonar, 148; sound intensity, 97-98; sounds: frequencies, 2, 29-30, 84-87; vampire bats, 85; whispering bats, 85, 87, 95

Beat frequency, 111-12

Beat note, 111-12

Bell Telephone Laboratories, 148

Biology, 10

Biophysics, 26-27, 56; defini- tion, 132

Birds, 148

"Bird Sonar," 148

Blindness, 10, 17, 20, 129-44; facial vision, 131, 135; orientation, 130; sound pro- ducers, 143-44. See also Vision

Blindness, 150

Blind people, 18, 26, 148, 150; activities, 131; echoes, 24, use of, 67; echolocation, 73, 110, 112, 143; orienta- tion, 44; orientation prob- lems, 100; orientation sounds, 107; sound dis- crimination, 104-5; sound fields, 51

Bowen, E. G., 147

Boys, C. v., 147

Brains, 10, 20, 56, 64

Brown bats, 85, 88; obstacles, 93. See also Bats

151

INDEX

Buoys, 58

Buddenbrock, W. von, 147

Burglar alarms, 51

California, 21

Cancellation, 44

Carthy, J. D., 148

Cathode-ray oscilloscope, 60

Chirping bats, 87

Chirps, 87

Clickers, 65-69, 72-73, 74-77; and echoes, 66-69; fre- quency ranges, 76

Communication, 19

Constant frequency pulses, 116

Constructive interference, 44, 48

Cornell University, 132

Cosmic rays, 35

Cotzin, Milton, 132, 133, 143

Creaking, 22, 23

Dallenbach, Karl M., 132,

133, 143 Darkness, 17, 27 David, Jr., E. E., 149 Dead spots, 50 Deafness, 148 Deep scattering layers, 110 Destructive interference, 44 Dijkgraaf, Sven, 31 Doppler effect, 113-16, 149 Duty cycle, 126-27

Ears, 36; bats, 28; human, 29, 45, 64, 107, 134, 147; inner mechanism, 19. See also Hearing

Earthquakes, 117; seismic waves, 116-17, 119

Echo detecting equipment, 36, 107

Echoes, 10, 24, 44, 52, 56, 148; acoustics of, 57-73; concentration of, 75-77; de- emphasized, 64; definition, 44; devices, 65-69, 70-73, 74-77, 107; effects of, 47; experiments, 65-69; func- tion, 45; magnitude, 61, 63; masked, 59; multiple, 113; objects, 73, small, 78-80; in prospecting, 116; reflections, 75-77; return, timing, 73; scattered, 70; suppression, 64-65; types, 81; use of, 10, 17, 29, 58, 116

Echo experts: in air, 26-32; in water, 18-26

Echolocating equipment, 36, 107

Echolocation, 21, 81, 149; in air, 108; bats, 29, 78, 90- 95, 99-100, 112; blind people, 148; definition, 18; devices for, 58; discrimina- tion problem, 119-20; fish- catching, 95-99; moving vehicle, 77-78; obstacles, 31; oil prospecting, 117; outer space, 108; porpoises, 22-26; underwater, 108; warnings, 73. See also Ob- stacle detection

"Echo-Location for the Blind," 150

Echo sounders, 58, 109; value of, 110-11

Eggers, Friedrich, 54, 55, 56

Electromagnetic waves, use of, 107-8

Electronic equipment, 10

Electronic hearing equipment, 30

Electronics, 149, 150

Electronic sonar, 149

Energy source, 149

152

INDEX

Equipment: communications, 3; detecting, 36; echolocat- ing, 107; electronic, 10; electronic hearing, 30; head- phones, 3; hydrophones, 111; oscilloscope, 60; seis- mograph, 117, 118; snoop- erscope viewer, 3; under- water hearing, 20-21; un- derwater loudspeaker, 111. See Instruments

Evolution, 104

Eyes, 36, 54, 147; see also Vision

Facial vision, 131, 135

False bottoms, 110, 116, 117

Fathometers, 58, 109

Feelers, 54

Fish, 148; auditory sense or- gans, 20; bladder, 25; de- tection of, 58; echoes, use of, 20; hearing, 19; noises of, 20

Fishermen, 58

Fletcher, H., 148

Florida, 21

Fluctuations, 48

Flute, 49

FM bats, 87, 95, 97, 113, 116, 125

Footsteps, 107, 138

Frequencies, 20, 25, 29-30, 49, 72, 84-87; high, 72

Fundamentals of Sonar, 148

Hearing: aquatic animals, 19; auditory nerve, 20; fish, 19, 20; humans, 19, 72, 99, 148, under water, 20, 111; losses, 148; sensitivity, 46; sound location, 140-42; wave length, 130. See also Ears

Hearing mechanism: in fish, 20; in humans, 19; in por- poises, 20, 24

High-frequency note, 47

Horseshoe bats, 84-85, 87, 116

Horton, J. W., 148

How Old Is the Earth, 118, 119

Hurley, P. M., 149

Hydrophones, 111

Icebergs, 108, 109

Incidence, angle of, 77

India, 21

Insects, 18, 148

Instruments, 17. See Equip- ment

Interactions, 35, 42-44, 48, 51

Interception, 18

Interference, 50, 149; con- structive, 44, 48; destruc- tive, 44

Jamming, 103, 148 Jurine, Charles, 28

Galambos, Robert, 29 Ganges River, 21 Griffin, Donald R., 148

Hairs, 53, 54 Harmonics, 49 Harvard University, 29

Lawrence, Barbara, 22, 23, 25, 99

Light, 18, 36, 42, 129, 130; velocity in water, 19; in water, 18-19

Light waves, 53, 80; reflec- tions, 70

Liquids, 40

153

INDEX

Listening in the Dark, 10, 25, 99, 104, 148

Man's World of Sound, 149

Marine animals, 110

Marine aquaria, 21

Marine mammals: sounds, 21. See Porpoises and Whales

Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Radiation Lab- oratory, 149

Maxim, Hiram, 108, 109

Mental telepathy, 137

Metronome, 75

M.I.T. See Massachusetts In- stitute of Technology

"Modern Sonar Systems," 149

Moehres, Franz P., 84

Molecules, 9, 19, 39, 40, 50, 119

Moon, 120; echoes, 147

"More about bat 'radar,' " 148

Multiple echoes, 113

Music, 49, 50

Musical instruments, 49

Natural selection, 104 Natural sonar, 148 Navigation, 2, 58; of animals,

148; bats, 2, 27, 32, 108;

radar, 147 Neutrinos, 35 Nuclear explosions, 117

Obstacle detection, 31, 132- 40; ears, 134-35; echoes, 138-40; skin, 133-34; sound, 136-40. See also Echolocation

Obstacles: for bats, 93; wires, 100-1

Oceanography, 147

Oceanography and Marine

Biology, 147 Oceans, 109; deep scattering

layers, 110; depth, 19, 40,

147; false bottoms, 110,

116, 117 Orientation, 10, 19, 27, 148 Orientation sounds, 84-87,

148; air to water, 97; bats,

2, 29-31, 84-87, 89-90,

97-98; frequencies, 84-87;

obstacles, 93-95; porpoises,

22-26 Outer space, 9, 120, 147 Oscilloscope, 60; screens, 120,

122 Owls, 27

Parabolic horn, 70-73

Pavia, 27

Physical Science Study Com- mittee, 7, 8, 10

Physics, 10

Piano, 49

Pierce, G. W., 2, 29

Pierce, J. R., 149

Pinging, 112

Plants, 9

Porpoises, 18, 20-26, 44; brains, 20; creaking, 22, 23; echolocation, 22-26, 99; hearing apparatus, 24; lan- guage, 21; objects, small, 104; Schevill experiment, 22-24; sonar, 148; sounds, 21, 22, frequency range of, 25-26

Probing signals, 18, 22, 23, 112

Psychology, 10

Radar, 18, 107-8, 120-27, 149; uses of, 120; wave length, 121

154

INDEX

Radar System Engineering,

149 Radioactivity, 149 Radio waves, 18, 35-36, 122 Rayleigh scattering, 125 Reflection, 75-77; angle of,

77; specular, 69, 77 Reinforcement, 44 Reverberations, 44, 45, 47, 55,

57, 113. See Echoes Ridenour, L. N., 149 Ripple tank, 49, 52, 69, 73 Rummell, J. A., 149

Scallops, 147 Scattering, Rayleigh, 125 Schevill, Barbara Lawrence,

22; William, 22, 23, 25, 99 Sciences, 26-27, 56, 132; inte- gration, 10 Science Study Series, 54 Scientific American Magazine,

148 Scientific history, 32 Scientific theory, 32-33 Seismic waves, 116-17, 149 Seismograph, 117, 118 Sense extenders, 51 Sense of obstacles, 131-40 Sense organs, 36, 54, 55, 56,

133, 147. See also Antennae,

Ears, Eyes, Hairs, Skin The Senses, 147 Shadows, 42 Sight: wave lengths, 130. See

also Eyes and Vision Sine waves, 36, 38 Skin, 133; pressure theory,

133-34 Soap Bubbles and the Forces

Which Mould Them, 54,

147 Solids, 40, 116 Sonar systems, 18, 107-8,

112-20; bats, 148; birds.

148; discrimination prob- lem, 109-10; Doppler effect, 113-16; echolocation: ice- bergs, 108-9, ocean bot- toms, 110, submarines. 111; electronic, 149; frequencies, 111-14, constant, 113-14, rapid, 112-13; marine life, 110-11; natural, 148 Sound: airborne, 19; corners, 42-43, 130; discrimination problem, 104-5; existence of, 40; frequencies, 38, 41, in bats, 29-31, 84-87, high,

29, low, 108, in porpoises, 25-26; indoors, 46-48; in- tensity, 45; marine animals, 20, 147; multiple reflections, 44; nature of, 9, 41; orienta- tion, 30, 148; outdoors, 46- 47, 48; pressures, 44; propa- gation, 149; reflections, 44, 75-77; scattered, 70; trans- mission in fish, 20; under- water, 20, 149; use of, 19, 118, 147; velocity, 44-45, in air, 65, measurements, 73-75, in sea water, 111, in water, 19, 24-25; wave lengths, 41-42, in water, 24, 25

Sound boundaries, 19-20, 40,

97 Sound-detecting equipment, 18,

19, 107-8, 112-20 Sound detection, 140-43 Sound energy, 19-20, 30 Sound-generating devices, 150 Sound theory, 134 Space, 40 Spallanzani, Lazzaro, 27-29,

30, 32, 87, 88, 108 Spallanzani's bat problem, 29-

30, 109 Specular reflection, 80 Speech, 49, 50, 148

155

INDEX

Speech and Hearing in Com- munication, 148 Standing waves, 49, 50, 51, 52;

patterns, 51 Stop watch, 74 Submarines, 17, 109, 111, 112,

113 Supa, Michael, 132, 133, 143 Suppressor mechanism, 64-65 Surface tension, 53, 147 Surface waves, 53; use of, 54

Tape recorder, 46, 47, 48, 50; reversed playback technique,

62-64, 66 A Textbook of Radar, 147 Time, 36; measurement of,

149 Titanic, 108

Underwater hearing equip- ment, 20-21, 22

Underwater loudspeaker, 111

Underwater sounds, 20, 149; discrimination, 110; energy of, 25; frequencies, 112

Underwater sound devices, 109, 110, 111

Vacuum, 40 Vampire bats, 85 Venus, 120 Violin, 49

Vision, 22; in water, 19. See also Blindness, Eyes, Sight Voltmeter, 47

Walter Reed Army Institute of

Research, 29 Washington, L., 150 Water, 18, 97, 116; clear, 19; dark, 18; sound in, 25; tur- bid, 18 Water beetles, 80 Water waves, 53, 69, 80 Wave lengths, 25, 26, 29, 41- 42; frequency, 72; under- water, 112 Wave motions, 9; property of,

36, 69 Waves: electromagnetic, 107- 8;light, 53,70, 80; radio, 18, 35-36, 122; scattering of, 80; seismic, 149; sine, 36, 38; sound, 18, 53, 149; sur- face, 53; use of, 17; water, 53, 69, 80 Whales, 18, 148; brains, 20; hearing, 19; noises of, 20 Whirligig water beetle, 53-56 Whispering bats, 85, 87, 95 Witcher, C. M., 150 Woods Hole, Mass., 22 Woods Hole Oceanographic

Institution, 22 World War 11, 111, 120, 121, 149

X-rays, 119

Zahl, P. A., 150 Zoology, 10

156

MARINE

BIOLOGICAL

UBORATORY

LIBRARY

WOODS HOLE. MASS. W. H. 0. L

ECHOES OF BATS AND MEN

In 1938, Donald R. Griffin, then a senior at Harvard, took a cage of bats to the physics building, where one of the first laboratories for detecting ultrasonic sounds had been set up. There, for the first time, the high-pitched clicks by which bats navigate were heard. This pioneer experiment has touched off ever-widening re- search in physical biology, research which has included the investi- gation of the navigating techniques of animals. Echoes oi Bats and Men is a brilliant report on how studies of bats, porpoises, and whirligig beetles and of electronic radar and sonar are now expand- ing man's understanding of physics. Dr. Griffin shows how this knowledge may be applied to help the blind "see."

^^^^^^^^^^^^^^V j.JiM^'mL

^^^T^cienc^fuoj^Series is part

^^^^^^^^^^BlW »nil!jLJo!

of a dramatic new program for

the teaching and study of

physics, originated recently by

[^■p^^mH^^B "^is^ ^M^B^^Pll

distinguished American scientists

1 W*ii^^r^^^!^^^^_^.:^^^^^^^^B

and educators meeting at the

Massachusetts Institute of

I^^H^^ jV'^HI

Technology. This Series of

up-to date, authoritative, and

1 ^^Rh!Bv. ^^h ^^^^KtiJkS\

readable science books is

^^^^B ^^^^^^^m jt ^^^> i^S^^^S^^K ^^^^^^^^^^^^^^^1

prepared under the direction of

■■■■ «■■■■* »^j0» .Jitiit 4«HBwVlv ■■■■■■■■■■RH

the Fhysicd Science Study

Donald R. Griffin was born

Committee of Educational

in 191 5 in Southampton, N. Y.

Services Incorporated and is

He grew up mainly around

published in co-operation with

Cape Cod and was educated

Doubleday Anchor Books and

at Phillips Academy, Andover,

Wesleyan Universit) .

Mass., and at Harvard

University. Dr. Griffin taught

physiology and zoology at

Cornell University until 1953,

and since then he has been

Professor of Zoology at

f r\ n 1

Harvard. His widely acclaimed

book Listeiung in the Dnrk

1 1

was published

V^f^y

in 1958.

A DOUBLEDAY ANCHOR ORIGINAL

L