The Cambridge Manuals of Science and Literature

HEREDITY

CAMBRIDGE UNIVERSITY PRESS

ftOlltttin: FETTER LANE, E.G.

C. F. CLAY, Managkr

CHtjinbuigb : loo, PRINCES STREET

ILontion: WILLIAM WESLEY & SON, 28, ESSEX STREET, STRAND

JSerlin: A. ASHER AND CO.

ILeipMg: F. A. BROCKHAUS

ip.cto ]imi;: G. P. PUTNAM'S SONS

33ombag nntj dakutta: MACMILLAN AND Co., Ltd.

A// rights reserved

QH DCS

First Edition 1910

Reprinted 1911

Second Edition 1912

/FzV/5 /)4(? exception of the coat of arms at the foot, the design on the title page Is a reproduction of one used by the earliest known Cambridge printer John Slberch 1 5 2 1

PREFACE

IN a book of the size to which the Cambridge Manuals of Science and Literature are limited, it is plainly impossible to treat in detail every aspect of a subject like Heredity. One of the chief difficulties, therefore, which I have encountered in preparing this little book has been to decide what to leave out. To some it will doubtless seem that parts of the subject have been treated too fully, and other important branches omitted or barely mentioned, but my aim has been to give the reader a sketch of the most important lines in which recent advances have been made. There are many excellent works dealing with the older theories and in this subject age is measured by very few years, but our knowledge has increased so greatly and is still progressing so quickly that books become out of date almost as soon as they are published. My attempt, then, has been to deal chiefly with the quite modern developments of the subject, and in order that the reader who is not very familiar with the matter may feel he is on fairly sure ground,

vi PREFACE

and not confuse fact with speculation, I have tried to avoid purely speculative questions in the body of the book, and have devoted a few pages to one of the most interesting of these in an appendix, together with a historical summary of Theories of Heredity. There are many, of course, who will regard parts of the chapters dealing with Mendelism as consisting largely of speculation ; I can only reply that I regard the facts referred to as established, and the theoretical deductions from them as the only ones that have yet been offered which can fit them adequately.

No attempt has been made to quote authorities for every statement, but a list of books and papers is given in which a further account will be found of the subjects treated. The numbers in square brackets [ ] in the text refer to this list. I have also followed the example set by Mr Lock in providing a glossary of unfamiliar terms of which the use has been unavoidable.

For the chapter on Statistical Study of Inherit- ance my chief sources of information have been Prof. Pearson's Grammar of Science and his numerous papers on the subject published by the Royal Society and in Biometriha. I have tried to summarise in words the results of his work, and of that of other workers on similar lines, and if the inadequacy of my mathematical knowledge has led me into any serious errors in the attempt, I owe them my apologies.

PREFACE vii

Chiefly of course I am indebted to Prof. Bateson's recent work on Mendel's Principles of Heredity as the most complete and authoritative account of the subject, from the acknowledged leader of the Men- delian school. For permission to reproduce several figures from this book I tender my thanks. In addition to information from many original papers, I have also not hesitated to make use of Lock's Recent Progress in the Study of Variation, Heredity and Evolution, Thomson's Heredity, and some other books dealing with the general aspects of the subject ; to these authors, and to several friends who have been kind enough to give me written information on matters with which they are especially conversant, I wish to record my indebtedness. I wish also especially to thank Mrs A. C. Seward for drawing the sections of Primula reproduced in fig. 10 (p. 81).

L. DONCASTEPu

Cambridge, June 1910.

CONTENTS

CHAP.

I. Introduction. Relation of Heredity to other branches of

knowledge.— The questions to be answered . page 1

II. Variation. Occurrence and kinds of Variation. Con-

tinuous variation and methods of study. Discontinuous variation. Inborn and acquired characters . 7

III. The Causes of Variation. Mutation. Action of en-

vironment on body and germ-cells. Variation on cross- ing.— Relative importance of 'inherent' and 'acquired' characters 22

IV. The Statistical Study of Heredity. Two methods of

studying Heredity. —The biometrical method. Corre- lation and Regression. Parental correlation and the Law of Ancestral Heredity. Heredity in 'pure lines' (Johannsen etc.). Inheritance of 'mental and moral' characters in Man. Eugenics .... 32

V. Mendelian Heredity. Mendel's Law illustrated.— Segre-

gation and Allelomorphism. Crosses concerning more than one pair of characters. Examples of Mendelian characters in Plants and Animals. Combs of Fowls. The Andalusian Fowl 52

VI. Mendelian Heredity {continued). The Inheritance

of Colour. Concurrence of two factors in the pro-

X CONTENTS

CHAP.

duction of Colour.— Colour in Animals ; some coloure 'epistatic' over others.— Flower-colours ; Reversion on crossing. The nature of albinism. More complex cases of interrelation ; Stocks, Primulas . . . 71

VII. Some Disputed Questions. Mendelian segregation.—

Inheritance of acquired characters. Indirect and ex- perimental evidence. Telegony. Maternal Impres- sion 85

VIII. Heredity in Man. Physical and Mental Characters.—

Diseases.— Mendelian Characters; Eye-colour, Brachy- dactyly, abnormalities of the Bye. Non-Mendelian characters; Skin and Hair-colour. Importance of Heredity in relation to Sociology . . . 101

IX. Heredity and Sex. Secondary sexual characters.— Dominance altered by Sex. Sex-limited inheritance; the Currant Moth, Fowls, the &j DrosophUa.— Sex-limited affections in Man ; Colour-blindness, Night-blindness and Haemophilia. Sex inherited as a Mendelian character. Sexual Dimorphism 119

Appendix I. Historical Summary of Theories of Heredity. Lamarck.— Darwin and the Theory of Pangenesis.— Weismann's Theory of Germ-Plasm . . .135

Appendix II. The Material Basis of Inheritance. The Nucleus and Chromosomes as possible 'bearers' of Heredity. Behaviour of Chromosomes in Germ-cell formation 145

Literature List l-j'

Glossary 1^^

Index ^^^

CHAPTER I

INTRODUCTION

During the whole history of scientific enquiry, one of the most fascinating and at the same time one of the most baffling of the problems which confront mankind has been the cause of the resemblances and differences between parents and children. In general, the facts are common knowledge; the essence of Heredity and Variation is expressed in the proverbs ' Like begets like ' and * Nature never uses the same mould twice.' Yet clearly the two proverbs are con- tradictory, for if like really begets like Nature must use the same mould for all the members of a family. Our object therefore is to investigate, first, how the characters of a parent actually are distributed among the children, and how the offspring of the same parentage may differ among themselves ; and secondly, if possible, what is the mechanism by which the re- semblances and diversities are brought about.

These problems are interesting from various points of view. They attract us for their own sake, as does

D. I

2 HEREDITY [ch.

anything mysterious or unexplained ; tbey have a deep human and practical importance, for not only do they affect us all individually, but upon their solution depends, to an extent as yet only dimly realised, the answer to some of our most pressing social questions ; and finally they lie at the very root of all theories of organic evolution, so that they form " as it were the basis of philosophical biology. The relation of the study of Heredity and Variation to sociology must be left to a later chapter, but before proceeding further we must shortly consider its bearing on theories of evolution.

The fact of organic evolution is admitted by all schools of biology, but about the causes of the pro- cess and the manner in which it takes place there is still wide diversity of opinion. To some of the more important theories of evolution it will be necessary to refer again later, but however great may be the difference of opinion with regard to them, all biologists agree that evolution depends ultimately on Variation and Heredity. Darwin called his great book The Origin of Species because the unit step, so to speak, on the scale of evolution is the transition from one species to another. But if a species A is to give rise to a species B, in the first place some individuals of A must vary in the direction of B, and then the variation must be inherited, for otherwise no permanent change can take place. The differences with regard to the cause

I] INTRODUCTION 3

and method of evolution arise therefore partly from our ignorance of the laws of variation and heredity, and partly from different ideas as to the causes which lead to progression in certain directions rather than in others. This latter source of disagreement is to a large extent outside the province of this book, but the subjects of Heredity and Variation are so inti- mately bound together that one cannot be adequately treated without the other. If, however, we can come to any definite decision with regard to the nature of Heredity and Variation, we shall have made a long step towards understanding the method by which evolution has taken and is taking place.

One other point must be mentioned. The study of heredity brings us face to face with perhaps the most fundamental problem of biology the ultimate nature of living matter. For if an ovum, barely visible to the eye, or the much smaller spermatozoon which is visible only with high magnification, can bear potentially all the parental characters which may be inherited by the offspring, it is clear that any hypothesis of the nature of living matter must take these things into account ; and though we cannot unravel or even imagine it, we can at least get some idea of the amazing complexity of the substances which in thoughtless moments we group together under the single name of 'protoplasm.'

1—2

4 HEREDITY [ch.

We will now attempt, by means of a few examples, to illustrate some of the questions which must be answered, and some of the facts which must be brought into relation, by any consistent account of the process of heredity. A tall man on the average has taller children than a short man, but if all the sons of a number of tall men were measured, it would be found that they showed every gradation in height between the tallest and shortest ; some would be taller than the fathers, others shorter, but every gradation between them would occur. Also, if a tall man marries a short wife, the sons are neither all as tall as the father, nor divided sharply into a tall group and a short group ; again they make a graded series from short to tall. But if we cross a tall variety of the sweet-pea with a dwarf variety, all the offspring are as tall as the tall parent, and among the offspring of these crossed tails, some are tall and some short, but none are intermediate. Here then we get two distinct modes of inheritance, and also two kinds of variation ; in the first case the character varies in such a way that all intermediates are found between the extreme conditions, and in the second the individuals can be classified sharply into two groups. Again, we cross a white mouse or rabbit with a black one, and all the offspring may have the grey-brown colour of the wild animal we have pro- duced what is called reversion to the wild type, and

I] INTRODUCTION 6

have obtained a form different from either parent. But if we mate the same black parent with another white individual, it may happen that all the offspring are black, and instead of reverting to the wild form they all follow one parent. If either the greys or blacks produced in this way are mated together^ some of their young will be white ; although none of the children of the original white individual resembled their white parent in colour, yet the white has appeared again among the grandchildren after skipping a generation. In man, a colour-blind father rarely has colour-blind children, but some of his nephews and male grandchildren through the female line are usually affected ; that is to say, the disease appears in males but is transmitted by females.

It is clear from this short list of examples that there are a number of different forms of hereditary transmission, and our object must be, first to classify them into groups in which the behaviour is similar, and next to attempt to bring them under a common scheme. And it is also clear that the different kinds of heredity are associated with different kinds of variation ; for example variation in height in man is inherited diffferently from variation in colour-vision, and both differ fi'om variation of coat-colour in rabbits, in their inheritance.

A question of a different kind is the cause of inherited diff'erences, and whether differences due to

6 HEREDITY [ch. i

the action of circumstances are inherited. Does a man, for instance, who develops certain muscles by frequent use, or who injures his health by excessive drinking, have children with larger muscles or poorer health in consequence? The question is frequently answered in the affirmative, but it is part of the province of the study of heredity to investigate the matter, and in these and all other cases to decide not only whether a character is inherited, but, if it is, to what extent and in what manner it will appear in the offspring.

CHAPTER II

VARIATION

We have seen that the subjects of Heredity and Variation are so closely connected that one cannot be considered apart from the other, for without variation all the offspring of the same parents would be exactly alike, and the study of heredity would resolve itself into an investigation of the cause of this likeness. But the actual problem is much less simple ; it includes the questions how and why the members of a family may differ from one another, and according to what rules and by what means these differences are transmitted to later generations. In practice therefore the study of heredity is the study of the manner and cause of the inheritance of variations, and hence the nature ^of variation must be examined before enquiry into its transmission.

Before the time of Darwin variations were fre- quently regarded as abnormalities, inconvenient to the systematist and of relatively small importance. Every species was supposed to conform to the type

8 HEREDITY [ch.

originally created, and divergences from this type were regarded as imperfections. But it was obvious that there was always more or less fluctuation about the type in different individuals, and breeders of plants and animals made use of this want of uni- formity to select the best specimens and so to improve the race. The Natural Selection theory of Darwin and Wallace supposes that a process comparable with this takes place in nature, and so brings about the adaptations of natural species.

Of the causes which induce variation nothing definite was known, but Darwin's belief was generally accepted that it is due to changes in environment acting directly or indirectly on the organism. He regarded the action of such changes as cumulative through a number of generations, so that its effect in producing variation might not be visible until the change had acted on several generations. This belief was founded on the observation that animals bred in captivity appear to be much more variable than in the wild condition, and the changed conditions of life are supposed to induce the variation. But species in nature are not by any means subject to uniform environment, and thus their variability was ascribed to similar causes.

Darwin and Wallace pointed out that variation occurs in all parts of every species, that it appears to occur in every possible direction, and to every

a] VARIATION 9

extent from very small to considerable range. They therefore founded their theory on this type of variability rather than on the occurrence of con- siderable 'occasional variations' which are not connected with the type by a series of intermediates. It was not, however, until after the theory of Natural Selection had obtained general recognition, that any detailed study was undertaken of the actual frequency and extent of variation, and its mode of occurrence.

The accurate investigation of variation has thus been in progress only for some twenty or twenty-five years, and according to the methods adopted students have become divided into two somewhat distinct schools. One of these has devoted itself rather to the attempt to observe and classify the different kinds of variation, and the other, generally called the ' biometrician ' school, to measure its frequency and range. It will be convenient to consider the results obtained by the second method first

If a character is chosen which can be accurately measured, such as human stature, and a sufficiently large number of individuals are observed, it will commonly be found that there is considerable range of variation, and that every gradation in size occurs between the smallest and largest. Such variation is spoken of as 'continuous/ as opposed to 'discontinu- ous' Tariation in which individuals of two kinds occur,

10

HEREDITY

[CH.

which are not connected by intermediates. Further, in cases of continuous variation it will appear that one size is more common than any other, and, in the simplest cases, that the individuals are progressively rarer as the size of the structure considered diverges more and more from the most frequent value. The most frequent condition is named the ' mode,' and its

76

75

74

«73

72

71

70

«69

JC68

g67

•-■66

65

64

63

62

61

X

1

7\

79

^

71

>

jo^

-^^

69

68

•^

67

^

1

~^_§^

1

65^

'-I

6

^

i

61

/

1

/

1

1

'

_

_

I

300

400

500 600

700

800

900 100O

Fig. 1. Curve illustrating stature, the vertical scale representing heights above 60 inches, the horizontal scale numbers of indi- viduals who are supposed to be placed side by side in order of their height.

size the 'modal value' for the character. For ex- ample, if the heights of a large number of men were measured, it might be found that they ranged by every gradation from 60 to 7Q inches. If the measure- ments were taken to the nearest inch, it might then be found that a greater number had a stature of 68 inches than any other height, that the next most

II] VARIATION 11

frequent heights were 67 and 69 inches, and that the more the stature differed from 68 inches in either direction, the fewer would be the men having that measurement. This could be represented graphically by arranging vertical lines representing the heights of every man in order of their height; a line joining their tops would then rise rapidly at the lower end, would be nearly flat as it passed over the men having heights near the 'modal value' of 68 inches, and would rise again steeply to the exceptionally tall men at the upper end of the row (fig. 1). [13]^

A more instructive method of graphically repre- senting the distribution of variation is to take a base- line and divide it into equal parts, each representing an equal increment in the structure measured. From each division of the base-line a vertical line is drawn representing by its length the number of individuals having that measurement.

In the imaginary case taken above, the base-line would have 17 divisions representing successive heights of from 60 to 7Q inches; at each division a vertical line is drawn which by its length repre- sents the percentage of the population which have that height (fig. 2). By joining the tops of the per- pendiculars (ordinates) a curve, or more strictly a polygon, is obtained which graphically represents the

1 For references see the end of the Volume.

12

HEREDITY

[CH.

distribution of the variation among the population measured. The highest point of the curve represents the mode for the character, and the extremes of variation are where it touches the base-Hne. The more numerous are the subdivisions into wliich the

180 170

"

,

160 150

>

.

■«

\i

/

\

"2 140

/

\

gi30 O 120

,?

\

j

\

1

1

/

\

^ no

1

1

/

\

u 100

/

\

a;

O. 90

1

0)

\

/

11

>> 80

1

o

\ .

•o

\

g 70 3 60

1

«i

§

\

1

A

\

<U 50

i

/

V

(I. 40

1

/

t

\

30

i

f

f

\

K

20

/

'

,

1

<

10

i?

A

i^

\

__.

*

•*

•*

A

■^ 1

—5

-^

^^

'*

•X

-*

s

^

(h

a

2 6

3 5

4 6

5 5

S 5

! 5

i 0

3 6

0 6

1 62

63 C

i 6

5 6E

1 6

7 6

3 &

9 7

D 7

< t.

! 7

3 7

» 7v

> 7

3 77 78 79

Stature in Inches

Fig. 2. Curves showing distribution of stature in women (mothers) —dotted line ; and in men (fathers)— continuous liue. The curves approach the 'normal curve.' (Data from Pearson. )

variable character is classified, tlie more nearly the line joining the tops of the ordinates will approxi- mate to a smooth curve ; e.g. if the population were measured to the nearest quarter of an inch instead of to the nearest inch there would be four times

II] VARIATION 13

as many ordinates and the curve would be nearly smooth.

It is clear that a curve of this kind can be used for comparing the variability of different characters, for the greater the variability of the population the wider Avill be the base ; consequently the curve for a very variable character will be relatively low and wide, that for a slightly variable one measured in the same scale will be tall and steep. A curve of this kind, which is quite similar on either side of the longest perpendicular ('median,' representing the modal value), may be obtained by plotting any measurements which vary fortuitously around a most frequent value, and such a curve is called a ' normal curve.' For example, if a large number of beans in- cluding equal numbers of white ones and black ones were placed in a sack, and drawn out ten at a time without selection of colour, most frequently five white and five black would be drawn, less often six of one colour and four of the other, more rarely seven and three and so on to the rarest case of ten of one colour. If the numbers of white beans in a draw are plotted along the base-line, and the ordinates represent the number of draws for each combination, a polygon approaching the normal curve will be obtained. Variation which gives a normal curve when plotted in this way is spoken of as normal variation.

As mentioned above, the steepness of the curve is

14 HEREDITY [oh.

a measure of variability, and this can be expressed by taking a point in the curve, the perpendicular from which to the base-line divides the area, enclosed by the curve, the median and the base-line, into two equal parts. Or, differently expressed, the perpen- dicular divides the curve in such a way that the number of individuals between it and the mean is the same as that between it and the extreme. The distance of this perpendicular may be used as a measure of the variability of the character considered, for clearly the greater the variability (and thus the flatter the curve), the further this perpendicular will be from the median \

In many variable characters, the frequency of variation below the mode is not exactly equal to that above it, in which case the curve will be steeper on one side of the mode than on the other, and the average value for the character ('mean') will not be identical with the mode. For example, if the varia- tion in the number of children in a family were plotted in this way, the sizes of families would range from 0 to about 20, but the most frequent number would perhaps be four. Four would then be the

^ In practice, not this perpendicular, bat another rather further from the median is used, which for practical purposes is more con- venient. The distance of this perpendicular, measured in units of the horizontal scale, is called the 'standard deviation' and is regu- larly employed as a measure of variability.

11]

VARIATION

15

modal value, but the average or meau might be about six ; the curve would rise steeply to the mode, and fall away more gradually to the maximum number (fig. 3). Such a curve is described as 'skew.' In

\

500

•400

>> u

a g 300

IT V

200

1

\

1

1

\

\

]

1

)

\

:

N

\

j

1

\

\

I

S

s

\

!:-

«T-)

•r^.

f=!!

O 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 1? 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 2425

Size of Family Fig. 3. Curve showing distribution of size of 3837 families in America containing deaf-mutes. (After Schuster, ' Hereditary Deafness.' Biometrika, Vol. iv. 1906, p. 474.)

extreme cases the mode is at one end of the curve, when variation takes place only on one side of it, e.g. in the marsh-marigold, the most frequent number of 'petals' is five, but there may rarely be six, seven or eight, but practically never less than five, so that in plotting the frequency a ' half-curve ' is obtained (fig. 4).

16

HEREDITY

[CH.

Another rather frequent condition is that the curve has two maxima or modes (fig. 5), indicating that a large number of individuals have a low measure- ment, a less number are intermediate, and again a larger number have a higher measurement Species which vary in this way are called ' dimorphic/ or if

300

aoo

100

5 6 7 8

Eig. 4. ' Half-cnrve ' representing the number of ' petals ' on 416 flowers of the marsh-marigold {Caltha palustrls). (After de Vries.)

there are more than two peaks to the curve, 'poly- morphic' Further, it is possible that the two parts of the curve should be entirely separate, if inter- mediates between the low and high groups are completely wanting. This type of variation is spoken of as ' discontinuous ' in contrast to the ' continuous '

n]

VARIATION

17

variation hitherto considered. It is possible that dimorphic cases in which intermediates exist are really essentially discontinuous, but that the two groups into which the species is divided each exhibit continuous variation about the mode for the group, to such an extent tliat the higher members of one

140

ISO

120

110

100

90

80

70

60

50

40

30

20

10

i

^

It

IT

tX ^V

m 4A

rjZA li:

J X t A

4 t -.-' V

t t 2 ^

t \ J- X

t A^^ ^

r '^

Lengths of Forceps in Millimetres

Fig. 5. Curve with two modes, representing frequency of lengths of forceps of male Earwigs from the Fame Islands. (After Bateson.)

group overlap the lower of the other. For example, if the modal (most frequent) stature for a race of men were 68 inches, and for the women 62 inches, it might happen that on plotting a frequency curve for the stature of adults including both sexes, a curve D. 2

18 HEREDITY [ch.

would be obtained having two maxima, one near 62 and another near 68. Yet the stature might be a definite sexual character, and hence essentially as discontinuous as the sexes themselves. This distinc- tion between continuous and discontinuous variation may seem unimportant in itself, but when its in- heritance is considered the distinction becomes of the first importance.

Clearer examples of discontinuous variation are given by such characters as colour, or by organs which are repeated in series, such as vertebrae and ribs, the segments of a worm or the petals of a flower. When variation occurs in this latter group it is generally complete, so that the different forms are visibly discontinuous. In the case of colour in the skin or hair in animals, or petals of flowers, discontinuity is sometimes less apparent, and grading frequently occurs, but even in apparently graded cases the inheritance of the character may often reveal discontinuity. For example, a piebald animal might be thought to be intermediate between the fully-coloured and albino (white with no pigment), but breeding tests would at once show that piebald- ness was an independent character, which cannot be regarded as in any sense intermediate between the other two conditions except in general appearance. The same applies to such cases as the ' silver ' cat or rabbit, or the pale purple sweet-pea ; the cause of

II] VARIATION 19

the pale colour is entirely distinct from the cause of the absence of pigment in the white varieties of those species.

The recognition of the importance of discontinuity in variation, which we owe chiefly to the work of Bateson in England and De Vries in Holland, is one of the chief advances which the study of the subject has made since the time of Darwin.

One other distinction between different kinds of variation must be mentioned here, which will be discussed more fully in subsequent chapters. The kinds of variation mentioned above are all inborn, or inherent in the individual and to a great extent in- dependent of its manner of life. But it is well known that the continued use of an organ or structure, or the prolonged action upon it of some external stimulus, may alter its form or cause it to assume a condition different from that which it would have had if these influences had not acted. In general, an organ tends to adapt itself either to the uses to which it is put or to the action of the environment which surrounds it. The muscles of a limb used for strenuous work in- crease in size and strength, or a part of the skin continually exposed to bright light develops a deeper colour than if it is covered. The converse process is also true ; an organ which is not used or exposed to its normal stimuli tends to diminish, and become less

2—2

20 HEREDITY [ch.

adapted to the use to which it is normally put. Such characters as these, arising in response to a stimulus, and not appearing in its absence, are technically called ' asguired characters,' a phrase which it will be necessary to use rather frequently in the following pages. As a rule, such 'acquired characters' are adaptive, that is, they render the organism or structure better fitted to its surroundings than if they had not been developed. The older students of heredity never doubted that these acquired characters were inherited as strongly as the inborn characters discussed above, but since the publication of Weismann's theory of heredity (see Appendix I) with the great body of evidence which he has collected on the other side, opinion has turned increasingly towards the belief that acquired variations are not transmitted. Weismann regards the germ-cells as essentially distinct from the rest of the body, so that acquired modifications of the body cannot be transmitted because the germ-cells are not affected. The germ-cells collectively, or rather that part of them which is concerned with the transmission of hereditary characters, he calls ' germ-plasm,' the rest of the body consisting of ' body-plasm ' (or * soma '), and he regards * acquired ' modifications as affecting body-plasm only. A developing germ-cell gives origin to both germ-plasm and body-plasm of a new

Ii] VARIATION 21

individual, and hence characters borne by the germ- plasm appear in the body ; but since body-plasm cannot be converted into germ-plasm, modifications of the body cannot be transmitted to offspring.

The possible inheritance of acquired characters is treated more fully in a later chapter.

r

CHAPTER III

THE CAUSES OP VARIATION

In the last chapter the distinction has been explained between continuous and discontinuous variation ; some confusion has however arisen with regard to the terms used in describing these conditions. Continuous variation about a mean (or more accur- ately modal) condition is sometimes spoken of as ' fluctuation/ but as will be seen below this kind of variability probably includes two very distinct groups of facts. It may include inherent variability arising in the germ-cells, or it may include differences in the adult condition having their origin in different effects of environment during growth. Some writers have used the word ' fluctuation ' for this latter condition only.

Discontinuous variation is sometimes called 'mutation,' a word which also has been used in several senses. It may mean the appearance of a form varying discontinuously from the type, or it may be applied to the discontinuous character itself.

OH. Ill] CAUSES OF VARIATION 23

A more serious source of confusion is that the term is used by some to denote any discontinuous variation arising ' spontaneously,' by others for cases in which the variety differs from the type in several apparently distinct characters, and not only in one, so that the new form constitutes an ' elementary species.' Since in studying heredity it is usually important to con- sider distinct characters separately, it may be per- missible to use the word for the origin of a form differing recognisably from the type and not connected with it by true intermediates.

It has already been pointed out that very little is accurately known about the causes of variation, and it is not impossible that the different forms of varia- tion have different origins. Most writers agree that the ultimate cause must lie in the action of environ- ment in some form, but as Darwin clearly stated in the Origin of Species the environment may act directly or indirectly. In variation of size for ex- ample, it is clear that the supply of nourishment, etc., during growth may have considerable influence on the size of the adult, and such variation will commonly be continuous owing to the evenly graded action on different individuals. In these cases the action is direct. If, however, Weismann's theory of germ-plasm and body-plasm is correct, such action may affect only the body and not be transmitted to off- spring. It is also possible that the germ-cells may

24 HEREDITY [ch.

be indirectly affected, giving rise to variation in the offspring; in such a case, however, there is no necessity that the effect on the offspring should be in any way similar to the direct effect of the conditions on the parent. Nothing is know^n of the nature of possible effects of environment on the germ-cells ; the action may possibly be effective immediately and give rise to variability in the next generation, or it may be that the effects are cumulative and only cause visible changes after several generations have been exposed to the same influences. Galton [13] suggested that the organism may have a certain * stability,' but that influences acting for several generations may have a cumulative effect which will gradually alter the equilibrium until it is finally upset and falls into a new condition of stability, giving rise to an apparently sudden variation. A chemical analogy may make this clearer. If litmus is added to an alkaline solution its colour will be blue. Acid may now be added drop by drop to neutralize the alkali, and suddenly, when the solution becomes acid, the litmus turns red. Examples of variation of which this may possibly be an analogy will be given below.

With regard to the action of environment on the body many facts are known, but it is not certain that they really have any bearing on the question of the origin of variation. For variations so produced are 'acquired characters,' and in many cases at least

Ill] CAUSES OF VARIATION 25

there is no evidence that they are inherited. For example, many butterflies have two generations in the year, one of which lives through its whole life- history in the summer and the other passes the winter as a pupa (chrysalis). In some cases the two genera- tions are strikingly different, and it has been shown that by freezing the pupae of the summer brood at the right stage, specimens like the spring brood can be obtained. The difference between the two genera- tions is thus due to the action of cold on the pupa. But the two forms regularly alternate in nature and the effects of cold are not inherited. In plants, some species produce quite different leaves according to whether they are grown in water or in dry soil, but the conditions act on the individual, and do not affect its progeny. In such a case, what is inherited is the faculty of making a certain definite response to definite conditions, and this faculty is present whether the conditions operate or not. In man such diseases as tuberculosis are commonly called hereditary ; this however does not mean that the child has the disease because his parent had it, but that the parent had a constitution liable to that disease, and the child inherits a similar constitutional liability. If the parent had never been exposed to infection the child would still inherit the liability, for what is transmitted is not the disease or its effects, but the faculty of acquiring it if exposed. It will be found

26 HEREDITY [ch.

that most cases which at first sight seem to support the theory of the inheritance of acquired characters are equally explicable in the view that both parent and offspring are susceptible to the action of the ex- ternal factor ; what is inherited is not the character acquired, but the innate power of acquiring it.

But it is always possible that some_ forms of external conditions may act on both the body-cells and germ-cells concurrently, and produce similar efiects in each. For example, it may happen that extremes of temperature produce striking colour- variations in certain butterflies. Weismann has pointed out that, according to his theory, in a developing butterfly the determinants for producing colour not only exist in the germ-cells which will transmit the character to the offspring, but also in the embryonic cells of the body which go to produce the coloured parts of the perfect insect. If extremes of heat or cold cause changes in the colour-deter- minants in the developing wings, so that abnormal colours result, it is possible that the determinants in the germ-cells which transmit the colour-pattern to the next generation will be similarly modified, so that the offspring will show similar abnormalities. This would not be the transmission of an 'acquired character ' in the strict sense of the expression, but the simultaneous modification of body and germ-cells in the same manner.

Ill] CAUSES OF VARIATION 27

But as mentioned above, it is possible that the same factor acting on body and germ-cells may pro- duce different results in the two cases, so that the individual on which the influences have acted may show one modification and its offspring another. It is also possible that not aU the germ-cells will be affected alike, and so among the progeny some will show modification and others not, or some may be differently affected from others ; for the conditions of stability of different germ-cells may conceivably be different. Certain experiments on insects give reason for supposing that this is so. The results obtained by Fischer, Standfuss and others from exposing pupae of butterflies and moths to abnormal temperatures, while not entirely concordant among themselves, on the whole indicate that moderate degrees of heat and cold tend to alter in the same way the whole batch of insects treate<l, often in the direction of varieties of the species naturally occurring in warmer or colder climates. But excessive heat or cold causes extreme variations among only a small proportion of the insects treated, and among the offspring of these ab- normal specimens only a small fraction are abnormal, and some of these have not the same abnormality as the parents. These observations, together with the fact that the variations produced by heat, cold, and other disturbing factors, may all be similar, suggest that extreme conditions may upset the stability of the type, causing abnormalities to appear.

28 HEREDITY [CH.

and that some of the germ-cells may also be altered, but not necessarily in the same manner as the body- cells.

An American zoologist, Tower, describes the pro- duction of mutations by the action of environment in a beetle (Leptinotarsa). In nature he found about one such variation among 6000 specimens ; when bred in captivity -they were more frequent, but when the fuU-grown beetles were exposed to extremes of heat, humidity, etc., during the maturation of the eggs, the offspring may include a large proportion (over 80 per cent.) of * mutations.' These were of several distinct kinds, like those rarely found in nature, and when bred together they are stated to breed true. In this case the abnormal conditions produced no effect on the individuals exposed to them, for they already had their final form, but as their eggs were matured under these conditions the action took effect on the eggs, and mutation resulted among the offspring. When part of the eggs of an individual were matured under abnormal, another part under normal con- ditions, mutation occurred only among the offspring in the first case, all the beetles in the second being normal. It should be noted that as in the experi- ments with butterflies the effect of changed conditions was not specific ; the same conditions may produce more than one kind of mutation in the same batch of eggs, and some eggs were not affected at all. In both cases the abnormal environment seems to upset

Ill] CAUSES OF VARIATION 29

the equilibrium, but the eifects may differ in different individuals. It is the nature of the organism or germ-cell affected which determines whether and to what extent the change shall take place ; the environment merely supplies the stimulus.

It will be seen that our knowledge of the causes of variation, in so far as these are connected with environment, is very incomplete and unsatisfactory, for although it is fairly clear that conditions may sometimes disturb the equilibrium of the germ-ceUs and provide a stimulus to variation, yet we have no knowledge of the way in which the stimulus acts and can make no prediction as to the direction the variation will take. Before leaving the subject, one other cause of variability must be mentioned the effect of crossing different races in producing varia- tion, it frequently happens that the result of crossing distinct races is that the crossed individuals differ from either parent ; sometimes in the direction of increased vigour, as was pointed out by Darwin, and other more recent observers ; sometimes by the development of characters apparently not possessed by either parent, as in the case of 'reversion on crossing.' The cause of this latter phenomenon will be discussed in a later chapter. In the subsequent generations from the cross great diversity may often appear, and Darwin supposed that the mingling of two distinct germinal stocks had an effect in dis-

30 HEREDITY [ch.

turbing the equilibrium similar to tbat produced by change of environment. To some extent this is doubtless true, but recent developments of the theory of heredity have afforded a more exact ex- planation, m the recombination of the different characters of the two races which are crossed. A fuller account of 'variation induced by crossing' must therefore be postponed until the principles of heredity have been discussed.

One further question should be mentioned before proceeding to the subject of heredity, namely, the relative importance of 'inherent' and 'acquired' characters in making up the sum of characters of a mature individual It is often assumed, especially in human cases, that the environment has a prepon- derating influence in shaping the individual. In a certain sense this is true, for many characters can only develop in a suitable environment ; muscles must be exercised to be properly formed and the mind cannot develop its full powers if it is never used. But the study of variation leads inevitably to the conclusion that the inherent characteristics are all-important, and that the effect of environment is not much more than to give them opportunity to develop. This is perhaps most impressively seen in the case of 'identical twins,' as has been shown by Galton [12]. There is reason to believe that such twins are produced by the division of one ovum, and

Ill] CAUSES OF VARIATION 31

even if exposed to different conditions they remain through life much more alike than ordinary brothers who may be brought up under precisely similar surroundings. The same fact is still further em- phasised by the study of heredity.

CHAPTER IV

THE STATISTICAL STUDY OF HEREDITY

In studying heredity, either of two methods may be adopted. We may either choose a character and observe or measure its development in a large number of parents and in their childi'en, and so deduce the average extent of resemblance between parents and children for that character ; or we may consider a number of individual cases separately, and endeavour to discover the manner in which the character appears in the children who have parents or ancestors pos- sessing it. With regard to the first method Prof. Pearson has written * We must proceed from inheri- tance in the mass to inheritance in narrower and narrower classes, rather than attempt to build up general rules on the observation of individual in- stances.' And ', . .the very nature of the distribution ...seems to indicate that we are dealing with that sphere of indefinitely numerous small causes, which in so many other instances has shown itself only amenable to the calculus of chance, and not to

OH. IV] STATISTICAL STUDY 33

the analysis of the individual instance' [25, 'JNIath. Contrib. III.' Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. A, 1896, p. 255]. The second method on the other hand has been used in cases where the causes of variation appear to be few and definite, and seeks to isolate these causes. The first method is thus clearly adapted especially to characters which vary continuously and which can be measured ; the second to characters which vary discontinuously and can be sharply separated into classes. The first method gives on the whole the average intensity of inheritance, but little information with regard to its probable development in individual cases ; the second attempts to answer the question in what manner the character wiU be distributed among the offspring in any family.

The founder of the modern statistical, or as it is now often called, the biometrical study of heredity was Sir Francis Galton, and its leading exponents have been Professor Karl Pearson and the late Professor Weldon. In this chapter an attempt will be made to explain the fundamental principles on which the biometric methods rest, and to outline the chief results obtained ; the methods themselves frequently require mathematics of an advanced order, and for the study of them the reader is referred to the books and papers dealing with the subject mentioned in the bibliography.

It has already been seen that in the case of a

D. 3

34 HEREDITY [ch.

character which varies continuously about a mean or mode, the greater the divergence from the mode in either direction, the fewer will be the individuals showing that divergence. In the case of human stature, if the modal height of a population is 68 inches, there will be fewer men of 64 or 72 inches than of 66 or 70, and still fewer of 63 or 73 inches. If now the sons of all the men having a given diver- gence were measured, and it were found that they averaged as gi'eat a divergence from the mode as their fathers, it is clear that on the average the height of the sons would equal that of their fathers. This does not mean that every son would exactly resemble his father in stature, but the sons would vary about the paternal stature equally above and below it, and when plotted in a curve their statures would make a curve having the paternal stature as its mode. The average stature of the sons would then be completely determined by the stature of the fathers. If on the other hand the stature of the father had no relation with that of his sons, it is clear that the statures of the sons of fathers of any height would vary about the mean of the general population considered. In practice it is found that the modal value for sons of fathers of a given height is between the height of their fathers and the mode of the general population. That is to say, if the fathers diverge a given amount from the general mode, their

IV] STATISTICAL STUDY 35

sons will on the average diverge less ; they will vary about a modal value lying between the general mode and the fathers' measurement. This fact is called * regression.' It sometimes seems paradoxical to those who have not considered it that the mean deviation of children from the general mode is always less than that of their parents. But of course it does not mean that all sons of tall fathers will be shorter than their fathers ; some will be as tall or taller, but the sons of a number of fathers of given stature will vary about a mode lying between the fathers' stature and the mode of the whole population.

Now it is plain that the amount of regression is a measure of the intensity of inheritance ; if the modes for sons of fathers of every deviation have deviations nearly as great as those of the fathers, the intensity of inheritance would be high ; if the modes for the sons deviate but slightly from the general mode, whatever be the deviation of the fathers, the intensity would be low. A deJQnite case may make this clearer. Suppose the modal stature of the population is 68 inches ; it might then be found that for fathers of 64 inches (i.e. deviating 4 inches below), the height of the sons ranged from 61 to 72 inches. If, however, their modal value had a deviation only slightly less than the fathers' deviation, say with a mode at 65 inches, the regression would be slight and the intensity of inheritance high ; if the sons' mode had a deviation

3—2

36 HEREDITY [ch.

much less than the fathers', say at 67 inches, the regression on the general mean would be considerable and the intensity of inheritance low. If then we can find means of determining the ratio between the deviation of sons in general and the deviation of their parents, we shall have a measure of the intensity of inheritance for the character considered. This ratio is called the 'coefficient of correlation' between father and son for that character. It should be noticed that correlation simply means that two quantities vary in relation to each other ; the corre- lation between parents and children is a convenient method of estimating the intensity of inheritance, but correlation exists between any two related variables, e.g. between the measurements of two limbs in the same individual, such as an arm and a leg, or between the numbers obtained in successive throws of dice, if not all the dice are picked up for the second throw. The correlation between the same measurement in brothers may be used as a measure of inheritance, for two brothers resemble each other more than two chance individuals because they are children of the same parents.

The principle of obtaining a coefficient of corre- lation between father and sons is as follows. It will be convenient to assume that the variability of the character considered is normal, i.e. that the frequency curve falls evenly on either side of the mode, so that

IV]

STATISTICAL STUDY

37

the mode is identical with the mean. Stature in inches may be taken as an example. A large number of fathers and a son of each are measured to the nearest inch ; it can then be found what is the average measurement of sons for fathers of each

«~ u §^

3"-

go

W a e "

u u

60 61 62 63 64 65 63 67 68 69 70 71 ^2 73 74 75 76 77

k

\

\

r

\

U

^

;

\

)

^ )

^

\

Vj

S'

<\

;

"^

^

C

0

v..

<

D

\

'^

s

\

"v

^N*

J

\

V,

■F

\

\

\

\

B

^

60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77

Statures of Fathers Fig. 6. Diagram of correlation between fathers and sons.

height from the lowest value to the highest. It will be found that the mean deviation of the sons from the mean of the population is less than the deviation of the fathers for each class of fathers. The average ratio between the mean deviations of the sons to the

38 HEREDITY [ch.

deviations of the fathers is then the coefficient of correlation between father and son for this character^ This is more clearly seen in diagram form.

If a square is made with its sides divided into equal lengths corresponding to equal increments in stature from 60 to 76 inches, the top may represent the scale of statures of fathers and the side the scale of mean statures of sons for each class of fathers. If, then, there were complete correlation between fathers and sons, the mean stature of sons of fathers 62 inches high would be 62, of fathers of 63 inches, 63, of 64, 64 and so on. If on the other hand there were no correlation, the means of the sons of every class of father would be the mean of the population (68).

In the first case the line joining the points repre- senting the means of the sons would be a diagonal running from corner to corner (AB), in the second case a horizontal line running across the middle (CD). But if the correlation is between these extremes the line would lie between the diagonal and the horizontal (EF), and the greater the correlation the steeper would be the slope of EF. The steepness of this line is thus a measure of correlation, and since all these lines pass through 0 in the middle of the square, the

1 It is assumed throughout that the variahiHty of the sons is similar to that of the fathers. If their variability were different this would have to be allowed for. The variation is also assumed to be normal, so that the mode in each case coincides with the mean.

IV]

STATISTICAL STUDY

39

slope is measured by the size of the angle EOC. The angle made by the diagonal at 0 is 45°, the tangent of which is 1 (unity). If there were no correlation the angle would vanish, EF coinciding with CD, and the correlation coefficient would be 0. Intermediates are represented by the value of the tangent of the angle EOC. In practice it rarely happens that the points representing the means of the sons for each class of fathers lie in a perfectly straight line ; when they approach it closely the correlation is called 'linear'; when they depart from it considerably, it is called ' skew.'

Table of i/ntensitj/ of parental inheritance in different species. [Froin Pearson.)

Species

Character

Mean value

Numbej

of Pain

used

Man

Stature

•506

4886

Span

•459

4873

Forearm

•418

4866

Eye Colour

•495

4000

Horse

Coat Colour

•522

4350

Basset Hound

Coat Colour

•524

823

Greyhound Aphis

Coat Colour Eatio of right Antenna

•507 •439

9279 368

(Hyalopterus trirhodus) Water-flea

to Frontal Breadth

(non-sexual reproduction)

Ratio of Basal Joint of

466

96

(Daphnia magna)

Antenna to Body length (non-sexuaJ reproduction)

40 HEREDITY [ch.

Prof. Pearson and his collaborators have worked out the correlation between parent and child for a number of measurable characters in Man, Animals, and Plants, and they find that the numbers group themselves about a value not far from 0*48, varying from 0-42 to 0-52. That is to say, on the average the offspring deviate from the mean about half as much as the parent.

The parental correlation hitherto discussed has taken no account of the second parent, for if in- dividuals mate at random the one parent may be considered alone, and the second will on the average have the mean value for the general population. But it is clear that one may take for the parental value in each class the mean of the two parents (making allowance for any difference in measurement due to sex), and plot the means of the sons (or daughters) against the classes so produced. The value derived from taking the mean of father and mother is called the * mid-parent,' and the correlation so arrived at would give the measure of resemblance between children and their mid-parents. This is naturally higher than the correlation observed when only one parent is considered; for if both parents deviate in the same direction from the mode of the population, the children will average a gi'eater deviation than if only one does so, and still more than if one deviates in one direction, the other in the opposite. We thus

IV] STATISTICAL STUDY 41

obtain a measure of the amount contributed to the offspring by the two parents together, but even now we do not find the correlation complete (I'O) because the contributions from previous ancestors have also to be taken into account.

Galton was the first to introduce the idea of the * mid-parent,' and he went on to attempt to estimate the average contribution to the children from each generation of ancestors. Since the correlation be- tween offspring and mid-parent is not complete, part of the heritage, which is not visibly present in the parents, must be contributed from more distant ancestors. Galton concluded from the data he collected that on the average half the heritage of an individual may be taken as derived from the two parents, one quarter from the four grandparents, one eighth from the great grandparents, and so on, the whole series (I? h h tV ) adding up to unity. Pearson estimates the average correlation between offspring and one parent, as about '5, of offspring with a grandparent as '33, with a great grandparent as '22, the correlation coefficient with an ancestor of each generation being I of that of the next below ; these numbers, however, are not in any way comparable with Galton's series •5, "25, '125, etc. Galton attempted to estimate the amount of the heritage received from the 'mid- ancestor ' of each generation independently of what was received from other generations ; but in the

42 HEREDITY [ch.

metaphor of bequests of property, he calculated that of the total heritage of an individual, half on the average was bequeathed by the parents, one quarter by the grandparents direct to the grandchild and so on, Pearson's series '5, '33, '22 etc. gives the average measure of resemblance between children and an ancestor of each generation, which is clearly a totally different thing. From this series he has worked out figures corresponding to Galton's, making the series •6244, -1988, "0630, i.e. he finds that the parental bequest is greater and the ancestral bequests less than Galton estimated. From the results obtained first by Galton and later by Pearson has been formu- lated the 'Law of Ancestral Heredity,' which has been stated in various forms, perhaps the most general being 'the mean character of the ofispring can be calculated with the more exactness, the more extensive our knowledge of the corresponding characters of the Ancestry ' (Yule [44]). But it should be noted that there is an important diflbrence between Galton's original statement of the law, and the later statements of Prof. Pearson. Galton wrote that ' the two parents between them contribute on the average one-half of each inherited faculty, each of them contributing one-quarter. The four grandparents contribute between them one-quarter, or each of them one-sixteenth ; and so on.' He regarded this as a physiological statement of the way faculties

TV] STATISTICAL STUDY 43

were transmitted, while Pearson, in his later writings at least, regards the law simply as a statistical description of what is found when large numbers are observed in mass.

It has been mentioned that the characters which especially lend themselves to statistical treatment are those which vary continuously and which can be accurately measured, but Prof. Pearson has applied similar methods to discontinuous characters, which can be classified into groups but not measured, for example coat-colour in horses. He finds as the results of his enquiries that the inheritance of such characters can be stated in terms similar to those obtained with measurable characters, so that the principle of ancestral correlation leading up to the law of ancestral heredity may be applied to these characters also. But whatever may be the case with characters which vary continuously, it will be seen below that discontinuous characters are commonly alternative in their inheritance, i.e. there is no blending, but the ofispring exhibit one or other only ; and in some at least of these cases, the character of the offspring cannot be calculated with any more exactness if the ancestry is known than if it is not. Such instances show clearly that although the law may be statistically true when applied to considerable populations, it gives us no clue to the physiological

44 HEREDITY [ch.

processes which determine the transmission of char- acters from one generation to another.

Another argument that has been used against the physiological validity of the law of ancestral heredity is based on the work of Johannsen and others who have obtained results similar to his in other cases. Johannsen worked at the inheritance of weight of seeds in beans and in barley, and self-fertilised the plants investigated for a series of generations so as to isolate what he calls ' pure lines.' He found that in beans, for example, the seed-weights of a mixed population gave a normal frequency curve the weights varied continuously and evenly about a mean value. The beans on an individual plant when the flowers are self-fertilised also form a normal curve about a mean, but this mean is not necessarily identical with that of the race in general. If now the flowers on such an individual are self-fertilised, and the beans produced are sown, the mean weight of the beans op all the daughter plants will be identical with the mean of the beans on the parent, i.e. among the ofispring produced by self-fertilisation there is no regression towards the mean of the race. It thus makes no difiference whether large or small seeds are chosen within the pure livie ; the mean weight of the seeds on plants grown from the smallest and largest of the parental beans (seeds) is in each case equal to the

ivj STATISTICAL STUDY 46

parental mean. Selection, therefore, within the pure line has no effect in altering the mean weight of the seeds, for the differences in seed weight within the line are not inherited. The probable cause of this is that the differences between the seeds on a self- fertilised plant are due to the action of external circumstances ; the position of the beans in the pod or the position of the pods on the plant cause differences in the nutrition which allow some beans to grow larger than others. These differences are 'acquired characters,' and we have here additional evidence that such are not inherited. It is to variation of this type that the term ' fluctuation ' is applied by some authorities.

It is clear then that if selection is made among beans harvested from a mixed population, on the whole the larger beans will belong to pure lines having a higher mean, and thus selection for a few generations will isolate pure lines having a high value, and the mean of successive generations will rise until the largest pure lines have been isolated. Beyond that point further selection will have no effect. This is precisely the result arrived at by Prof. Pearson from a study of selection within a mixed population ; the mean will rise rapidly on the first selection, more slowly later, until in very few generations it reaches a point at which selection has no appreciable effect. Pearson calculates that if

46 HEREDITY [ch.

selection now ceases, the selected race will very slowly revert towards the mean of the general popu- lation. But, as has been seen, this conclusion is based on the assumption that continuous variation is due to the concurrent action of an indefinite number of small independent causes. If, however, Johannsen is correct, we may divide these causes into two classes : the causes which induce ' fluctuation ' as explained above, which agree with Pearson's requirements, and the cause or causes which give rise to the difference between one pure line and another. Now this second group may conceivably consist in a single factor of the nature of a small ' mutation,' and if so, by isolating the pure line this factor is also isolated, and no return towards the mean of the general population need take place. According to Johannsen this isolation can be efibcted in one generation by selecting the self-fertilised plants which have the highest average yield, instead of selecting the heaviest beans them- selves.

We thus obtain by experiments such as those of Johannsen a new conception of the possible nature of continuous variation ; it may be due partly to ' fluctua- tion' brought about by the action of environment and not inherited, partly to a series of small step- wise 'mutations,' each of which owing to fluctuation overlaps the next, and can only be isolated when it is possible to breed pure lines. It should be said that

IV] STATISTICAL STUDY 47

there is as yet no certainty that this account of continuous variation is sufficient to cover all cases ; it is a suggestion of possibility rather than a state- ment of fact.

We have seen that there is reason to believe that the Law of Ancestral Inheritance is true only when applied to a large number of individuals considered in mass, or, as it has been put, that it is a statistical rather than a physiological law. In individual cases it is not true that the offspring need be influenced by ancestors beyond the parents, but in other cases, as will be seen in dealing with Mendelian heredity, these ancestors have important effects, so that statistically it is possible to say what is the average influence of the ancestors of any generation upon the offspring. Now in cases where it is possible to define rigidly single characters, much more is learned from the physiological than from the statistical method, but where no such rigid separation of characters is possible the statistical law is the only one that can be applied. This is particularly the case in characters which vary continuously, or where the categories into which the character falls overlap one another, as for example in Johannsen's beans. Further, the statistical method is frequently the only one which is available when experiment is impossible and when our knowledge of the facts is based solely on numerical data from observed cases, and this of

48 HEREDITY [CH.

course applies especially to inheritance in Man, where experimental evidence is not available.

By collecting family histories of distinguished men, Galton showed long ago [15] that exceptional mental qualities were inherited; and this work has recently been much extended and made more definite by Professor Pearson and his school. It is commonly believed that exceptionally gifted men do not have distinguished sons, but this like many other popular beliefs is only partly true. It has been seen that if an individual deviates a certain amount from the general mean, his children will on the average deviate less, because when the whole ancestry is taken into account, the eflect of previous generations is to cause regression on the mean of the population. And since the theory of regression depends on the assumption that variation is due to the existence of a large number of inde- pendent causes acting concurrently, it is unlikely that among the limited number of offspring of one exceptional man any one child will unite in himself the same combination of factors as went to make up the father's character. Further, it is improbable that an unusually gifted man will marry a wife equally gifted in the same manner, and the mother's influence on the children is closely similar to that of the father. It cannot therefore be expected that all great men should have equally great sons, but they are far more

IV] STATISTICAL STUDY 49

likely to have exceptional sons than are mediocre men, and if the mother is also exceptional in the same direction this probability is greatly increased. In the last feAV years the intensity of inheritance in such characters has been given numerical ex- pression. Professor Pearson, after working out the statistical laws of inheritance in many physical char- acters of man, animals and plants, has applied the same methods to what are called the mental and moral attributes. Characters were chosen such as vivacity, popularity, conscientiousness, temper, ability, hand-writing, which were estimated by reports from school-teachers on the children in their schools ; and also intellectual ability as shown in university examinations or by the position in a public school at a particular age (Schuster [10])\ All these, when investigated by the same methods as were devised for the coat-colour of horses or eye-colour in man, are found to give results closely in accord with those obtained for physical features. The conclusion is therefore reached .that not only bodily characters, but also those of the mind are essentially determined by the hereditary endowment received from the parents. This result is of great importance practi-

^ In these characters the resemblance between parent and child cannot of course be estimated directly, but it has been pointed out above that the resemblance between brothers may be used as a test of the intensity of heredity.

D. 4

50 HEREDITY [CH

cally ; it shows how little room is left in the development of the individual for the effects of environment even on the intellect or mind in the broadest sense of the word ; no doubt the direction which intellectual development takes is to a con- siderable extent determined by circumstances, but the kind of mind is irrevocably decided before the child is bom. Still less is there room for the inheritance of the mental acquirements made by the individual during his life, and hence the hopes held out of improving the race by education and by special care of the dull or feeble-minded are illusory, except in so far as they improve the tradition. Just as the welfare of the race may be increased by an invention which is handed on from generation to generation, so the good effects of education or other improved conditions may be handed on, but this is not heredity. The father may educate his children because he himself was educated, but the mental powers of his children will be the same whether he had a good education or none\ And the effects of special care given to the weakly or feeble-minded may be absolutely harmful to the race, if the im- provement so effected leads to more frequent

1 Of course education is a necessary condition for the full develop- ment of the mental powers, hut at present we have no evidence that it can add potentialities not present at birth. The subject is more fully discussed in Chapters vii and viii.

IV] STATISTICAL STUDY 61

marriage among such unfortunates than would other- wise be the case, for then an increased number of defective cliildren may be born, and the race-average be lowered. Hence has arisen the study known as 'Eugenics,' the study, that is, of the methods by which the race may be improved both physically and mentally. The whole trend of the results obtained is that in order to produce exceptionally gifted men in both body and mind, those with high development of the characters desired should be encouraged to marry ; and that to prevent the production of the weakly and feeble-minded, the only method is to prevent such from having oflFspring. It is admitted that at present these things hardly come within * practical politics,' but there is little doubt that the nation which first finds a way to make them practical will in a very short time be the leader of the world.

4—2

CHAPTEE V

MENDELIAN HEREDITY

In the last chapter the distinction has more than once been referred to between the statistical rules of inheritance discovered by observing great numbers of cases taken together, and the physio- logical laws which determine the actual manner of transmission in individual cases. The province of the present chapter is to indicate the methods by which one at least of these physiological laws has been investigated, and the results to which such work has led. In studying this part of the subject it is necessary to consider, at least in the first place, characters which vary and are inherited discontinu- ously, so that they may be sharply marked into distinct categories. The foundation of the study was laid by Johann Gregor Mendel, a monk of the monastery of Briinn in Bohemia. His most important paper was published in 1866 [2], but perhaps owing to the fact that the biological world was then

CH.V] MENDELIAN HEREDITY 53

occupied almost solely with the discussion of the * Origin of Species,' his work attracted no attention at the time, and only became celebrated on its rediscovery in 1900. One cannot avoid speculating on the possible effects on biological thought, had the experiments and conclusions of his now famous contemporary ever come to the knowledge of Darwin.

The method which led Mendel to his great discovery was to experiment with plants exhibiting discontinuous characters, and to consider each char- acter separately. Previous workers in the same field had made many laborious experiments in crossing difterent races of plants or animals [7], but had always regarded the individual as the unit, and hence arose the belief that mongrels or hybrids were usually intermediate between the parents, resembling one in some features, the other in others, but with no regular rule ; and further, that when hybrids were bred together the oiFspring were often almost infinitely variable, extending in a series from some closely approaching one original parent through a diversity of intermediate or new forms to others like the second parent. So grew up the belief that the crossing of distinct races or breeds is a potent cause of variability, which, however, except when 'reversion on crossing' took place, seemed to fall under no ascertainable law.

54 HEREDITY [ch. v

Mendel's most important experiments were made with races of the edible pea, which he grew in the garden of his monastery. He found in peas several characters which vary and are inherited discon- tinuously, and he crossed together races which differed in one or more of such characters, but in the oflfspring and later generations he considered the distribution of each character by itself, quite apart from the other characters of the plant. As an example we may take the character height or tallness. Certain varieties of peas grow stems some six feet in height, others are short and do not exceed about two feet. The heights fluctuate about a mode, but the smallest individuals of one race (grown under proper conditions) are taller than the largest of the other, and each race breeds true. Similar tall and short races exist in the sweet-pea (fig. 7), the short race being called 'Cupid' sweet-peas. When the two races are crossed and reciprocal crosses give identical results the offspring are not intermediate but all are tall, perhaps taller than the tall parents. When now these hybrid tails are self-fertilised, among the plants produced some are tall and others short, but again none are intermediate. Mendel regarded the tallness or shortness as distinct alter- native characters, and since tallness alone appears in the fii'st cross, he spoke of it as ' dominant,' and the shortness, which disappeared when crossed with the

56 HEREDITY [ch.

dominant tallness, he called 'recessive. More recent work has indicated that a dominant character pos- sesses some factor which is absent in its recessive alternative ; in the present example the stem has the power of continued growth which is absent in the short pea. Dominance and recessiveness may thus be regarded as presence and absence respectively of the factor in question ; but since the presence or absence of the factor may often give rise to the appearance of an alternative pair of characters, such a pair have been named by Bateson a pair of 'allelomorphs.' When a tall pea is crossed with a short, the factor tallness is introduced from the tall parent, and thus all the offspring are tall. These are called the first filial generation, or more shortly the generation JPj. Wlien these hybrid (F^) tails are self-fertilised, their offspring (second filial or F2 generation) consist of tails and shorts. Now it has been seen that if the factor tallness is present it makes itself visible, and therefore the short peas in Fi should contain no tall factor. And in fact when self-fertilised, or fertilised with the original short stock, they give only short offspring for as many generations as the experiment has been carried to. The tall factor has thus apparently been completely eliminated from these short peas.

Fm-ther, Mendel found that among the tails in the Fi generation, some breed true to tallness when self-

vj MENDELIAN HEREDITY 67

fertilised, while others again give a mixture of tails and shorts. The whole result may be clearer in symbolic form. If T stands for the tall factor, t for its absence (shortness), the following results appear. (The "2Tt' in Fi will be explained immediately.)

Original parents

Txt

1 Tt

Ft

r TT

1 TT

2Tt

1

1

tt

1

Fs

1 1 1 TT 2Tt tt

1 tt

It is thus clear that among the offspring of the Fi (hybrid) generation, some (tt) have eliminated the taU factor altogether and show no difference from their short ancestors ; others (TT) have nothing but the tall factor and thus breed true to tallness ; and a third group, which Mendel found was twice as numerous as either of the others (therefore marked 2Tt), proved, by giving mixed offspring when selfed, that it is hybrid like its jPi parent.

The explanation offered by Mendel of these facts was as follows. The original tall plant produces germ-cells ('gametes') bearing taUness ; the short plant produces gametes bearing shortness (absence of tallness). The Fi (hybrid) thus contains both conditions ; its cells, resulting from the union of two gametes, may be regarded as double structures, con-

58 HEREDITY [ch.

taining- a double set of determinants for the various characters of the plant, one determinant of each pair being derived from the male parent, the other from the female. An individual produced by union of two germ-cells (gametes) and having this double character is called a ' zygote/ The F^ zygote thus contains a determinant for tallness derived from one parent, and a corresponding determinant in which the tall factor is absent derived from the second parent. Now Mendel's hypothesis to account for the observed facts was that although the zygote produced by union of tall-bearing and short-bearing gametes contains both factors, yet when this hybrid zygote gives rise to gametes, it produces some bearing tallness and others bearing shortness, but none bearing both determinants ; i.e. that the alternative characters segregate from each other in the forma- tion of the gametes, and that gametes bearing one or other of the two conditions are formed in equal numbers. Since large numbers of gametes of each kind are formed, and since they meet indiscriminately in fertilisation, a tall will equally often meet a tall or a short, and a short will equally often meet a tall and a short, and the combinations will thus be in the ratio of \TT, \Tt, UT, Ut, or ITT, 2Tt, \U. If this hypothesis is true, it can be tested by fertilising the Fi hybrid zygote with the pure parental types ; the Fi zygote produces equal numbers of T and t gametes,

v] MENDELIAN HEREDITY 69

the pure short race produces only t, so the offspring of the hybrid and the original short should give equal numbers of hybrid tails and pure shorts. Similarly the hybrid zygote crossed with the pure tall should give equal numbers of pure tails {TT) and hybrid tails {Tt), Mendel found that this expectation was in fact verified by experiment. The whole series may be made clearer by a diagram (p. 60), in which the zygotes are represented by squares, the gametes by circles.

The middle part of this diagram represents .the production of the Fi zygote and its ofispring when self-fertilised, producing equal numbers of T and t gametes (four of each being represented) and thus giving offspring in the ratio of \TT, 2Tt, lit; the sides of the diagram represent the results of crossing back the Fi zygote with the parental types TT and tt.

At this point it is necessary to explain certain convenient technical terms introduced by Bateson. It has already been mentioned that a pair of alter- native characters which segregate in the gametes, as described, are called allelomorphs. When an indi- vidual is produced by two gametes bearing different allelomorphs, so that it contains both members of a pair, it is called a ' heterozygote,' or is said to be * heterozygous ' in respect of the character considered, e.g., an individual of constitution Tt is heterozygous in respect of talluess. If it contains only one kind

60

[CH.

zy

-W

X

h

9 ^

-I

h-

H

to

s:

CD

5

00

CO

so

^

CO

S "^

o

CO

o

s-1 03 CO V

2r>

!3>

N

s

N

«a

uT

C3

uT

v] MENDELTAN HEREDITY 61

of allelomorph of a pair it is a * homozygote/ e,g. individuals of composition 2Tand tt are 'homozygous' for tallness and shortness respectively. As will be seen immediately, it is possible for an individual to be heterozygous for one pair of allelomorphs and homozygous for another. The essence of Mendel's theory is that owing to the segregation of allelomorphs from each other in the production of the gametes of a heterozygote, the homozygous offspring, when self- fertilised or mated with others of like constitution, breed true to the character in question irrespective of their ancestry. As far as observation can show, the homozygous individuals TT and tt in the genera- tion Fi breed as true to tallness or shortness as did their pure-bred grandparents, in spite of the fact that they are the offspring of a cross.

Hitherto the original parents have been considered as differing from each other in only one pair of alter- native characters (allelomorphs), but Mendel found that in the pea there were several such pairs of characters. For example, some races of peas have purple flowers, others white ; these behave quite similarly to tallness and shortness. The purple flower contains a factor lacking in the white ; when therefore purple is crossed with white, the purple colour is dominant and the heterozygote {F^ hybrid) is purple. Such a heterozygous purple if self-fertilised yields 75 per cent, of purple oflspring and 25 per cent.

62 HEREDITY [ch.

of white ; the whites and one in every three of the purples so produced are 'extracted' homozygotes, being pure for whiteness or purpleness respectively, and therefore breeding- true, while the remaining purples are heterozygous and when ' selfed ' will give both colours among their ofispring.

If now a tall purple-flowered pea is crossed with a short white-flowered, the heterozygous offspring will be tall with purple flowers, for both these characters are dominant. In the production of their gametes (pollen-cells and egg-cells) segregation will take place between tallness and shortness, and between purpleness and whiteness, but as these pairs of characters are totally independent of one another they may be associated in any combination as long as both members of a pair do not occur in the same gamete. Gametes will thus be produced of four kinds ; if P represents purple, p its absence (white) ; T tallness and t its absence (shortness), the gametes produced by an individual heterozygous in both characters will be PT,pT, Pt, pt, with equal numbers of each. Since these will meet one another at random in fertilisation, the F^ generation will consist of individuals (zygotes) made up of all possible com- binations of these four types of gametes, viz. in the proportion of 4.PpTt, 2PpTT, 2PPTt, \PPTT; 2Pptt, \PPtt ; 2p27Tt, \ppTT ; \pptt

Since purple is dominant over white and tall over

v] MENDELIAN HEREDITY 63

short, the first four types of zygote, which all contain both P and T, will be purple tails ; the next two containing P but no T will be purple short ; the two containing T but not P will be tall white, and the last with neither P nor T will be short white. The F2 offspring will thus appear in the ratio of 9 purple tall, 3 purple short, 3 white tall, 1 white short. Further, of the first group one will be homozygous in both characters (PPTT), four homozygous in one and heterozygous in the other (2PPTt, 2PpTT) and four heterozygous in both (PpTt). Of the remainder, one in each class will be homozygous in both char- acters, and the others heterozygous in one, the homozygous (pure) types being PPtt, ppTT and pptt.

It is clear then that by crossing two races which differ in two allelomorphic characters, and self- fertilising (or mating together) the crossed individuals, in the F^ generation a definite proportion of neiv pure combinations are produced. In the above example, by crossing tall purple with short white, in the second generation not only these types are produced, but also short purple and tall white, and by selecting the pure (homozygous) individuals pure races of these new types are immediately established. We thus obtain a new conception of organic char- acters, as factors which can be replaced by alternative characters without otherwise altering the constitution

64 HEREDITY [ch.

of the organism. The process is comparable with a chemical reaction, where one element may replace another in a compound ; for example, by mixing silver nitrate with sodium chloride, silver chloride and sodium nitrate are produced. Or a grosser analogy may be taken from bricks in a wall ; a red brick may be removed and replaced by a blue or a yellow one without altering the rest of the wall, and similarly in pea-plants by the process described white flowers may be replaced by purple, or yellow seed by green. After the fact of the segregation of allelomorphic characters in the production of the germ-cells of a heterozygote, the most striking result of Mendcljan investigation is this discovery of the independence of ohav^o.tf-.r'fi hftlnnffvpfy to different pairs.

That these results are not of merely academic interest is shown by the work of Prof. Biflen on wheat. Some valuable wheats are liable to the attacks of a fungus giving rise to the disease called ' rust,' other less valuable races are immune. Biffen has found that by crossing the two races together, fertilising the hybrids (Fi) among themselves, and selecting the homozygous plants in the Fo generation, wheat can be produced which combines the valuable features of one race with the immunity to rust of the other, and so a new and most useful variety of wheat is produced. This is only one out of many examples that could be given of the possibility of combining

V]

MENDELIAN HEREDITY

65

into one race of wheat the characters previously found in different varieties.

The chief reason that breeders of plants and animals believe that tlie race is permanently con- taminated by crossing different breeds is that commonly two breeds differ in several or many pairs of characters. If two pairs of allelomorphic

Fig. 8. A cob of Maize borne by an Fi plant from the cross smooth (starchy) seed x wrinkled (sugary) seed, fertilised with its own pollen, showing about three smooth (dominant) to one wrinkled (recessive) seeds on the same cob. (From Bateson.)

characters are combined in the heterozygote, we have seen that only one in sixteen of its offspring is homozygous for any particular combination ; if three characters, one in 64, if four characters, one in 256, so that it is clear that Mendel's method of consider- ing distinct characters separately must be followed, if

D. 5

66 HEREDITY [ch.

any rules are to be arrived at for the distribution of characters among the offspring of hybrids.

Before proceeding to consider some of the further applications of Mendelian inheritance, a few examples will be given of characters in animals and plants which are found to be inherited according to this law.

In plants, flower-colour, seed-colour (due to either seed-coat or the contained embryo) ; production of starch or sugar in seeds (maize, see fig. 8 in which both forms of seed are shown on the same cob) ; hairiness or smoothness (stocks. Lychnis, etc.); 'bearded' or 'beardless' ears (wheat); 'palm-leaf or 'fern-leaf (Primula) ; long or short styles ('pin-eye' and 'thrum- eye ' of Primula) ; pollen-shape, and also fertility or sterility of anthers (sweet-pea). Many other examples could be given ; it should be noted that several of these normally occur in nature, e.g. the two flower- types of the primrose.

In animals, coloured coat and albino (many mammals) ; and many other colour-characters in mammals and birds ; normal and long or ' Angora ' hair in rabbit, guinea-pig, etc. (some doubt as to completeness of segregation) ; comb-characters in fowls ; leg-feathering in pigeons ; horned and horn- less condition (sheep and cattle) ; colour-characters in moths, beetles, and snails. In man, several abnormal conditions, and presence or absence of brown pigment in the iris of the eye.

v] MENDELIAN HEREDITY 67

As in plants, several of these cases are not in any way connected with domestication, and the wide diversity of species and characters in which Mendelian inheritance has been discovered shows that the phe- nomena are not rare or exceptional, but universally distributed.

It has been mentioned that of a pair of allelo- morphic characters, one is regarded as containing some factor absent from the other, and it may be well to give an example of the kind of evidence that leads to this conclusion. In fowls there are three chief forms of comb; 'single' with a median serrated ridge, 'rose' with a broad upper surface covered with papillae, and 'pea' with a shape consisting essentially of three parallel low ridges. Rose and pea each behave as dominants to single, but when rose is crossed with pea a fourth type, 'walnut' results, which in the adult is swollen and dimpled, and, in the young at least, is crossed by a transverse band of bristles. In the Malay breed such ' walnut ' combs breed true, but when made by crossing ' rose ' by 'pea,' and mated together, the resulting chicks appear in the ratio of 9 walnut, 3 rose, 3 pea, 1 single. The appearance of singles in the F^ generation from pure rose by pure pea is explained by the ' presence and absence ' hypothesis. Rose (R) and pea (P) are each allelomorphic with their absence (r, p). A rose- combed bird is thus Rp, and a pea-combed rP,

B

D

Fig. 9. Types of combs in Fowls. A. Single Comb (cock). B. Pea Comb (cock). C. Pea Comb (hen). D. Eose Comb (cock). E. Walnut Comb (young cock). (From Bateson.)

CH. v] MENDELTAN HEREDITY 69

and the walnut combs produced by crossing them have constitution RrPp. They produce four kinds of germ-cells, RP, Rp, rP, rp, giving the normal ratio in i^a of 9 birds containing R and P, 3 with R and p, 3 with r and P, 1 rp. This rjJ, containing neither rose nor pea is single, which may be regarded as the normal comb with no other factor superposed upon it.

In conclusion, one further fact should be noted. Although the members of an allelomorphic pair differ from each other in that one contains a factor lacking in the other, and this present factor is commonly dominant over its absence, yet a number of cases are known in which the introduction of a factor from one parent only is not sufficient to cause its full develop- ment in the heterozygote. The crossed offspring are then different from both the parental types, and are commonly intermediate between them. But when such heterozygous forms are mated together or self- fertilised, both the homozygous parental types are produced in addition to the heterozygous form, as in the offspring of a heterozygous tall pea there occur homozygous tails and shorts in addition to hetero- zygous tails. The classical example of this condition is the blue Andalusian fowl. This breed cannot be bred true ; when blues are paired together about half the chickens are blues and the remainder evenly divided between blacks and dirty-whites. By many genera-

70 HEREDITY [ch. v

tions of selection breeders have tried without success to eliminate these black and white 'wasters/ but it remained for Bateson and Punnett to show that if a black and a white are paired together, only blues are produced. The two homozygotes are black and white respectively ; when these are paired together the single black factor introduced from one parent is insufficient to cause the crossed chicks to be full black, and a dilute black or 'blue' results. Such incomplete dominance, in which a single factor introduced from one parent is insufficient to bring about the same effect in the heterozygote that is produced by the 'double dose' present in the homozygote, has been observed in a number of cases, some of which must be referred to later.

CHAPTER VI

MENDELIAN HEREDITY (rontinued) The Inheritance of Colour

In the simple Mendelian cases discussed in the last chapter the separate allelomorphic pairs were described as wholly independent of one another, and in the manner of their inheritance this description is correct for allelomorphic pairs in general except in special cases, of which examples will be given later. But although allelomorphs of distinct pairs are in- herited independently, yet not infrequently they may react upon one another so as to give an apparently combined effect in the individual bearing them. This is especially, but by no means exclusively, seen in the colour-characters of animals and plants. In the list of examples- of Mendelian characters it was mentioned that coloured coat in animals or coloured flowers in plants behave as an alternative to whiteness (albinism, i.e. the absence of pigment). But further analysis shows that the appearance of colour depends upon the presence of at least two

n

HEREDITY

[CH,

factors, in the absence of either of which no colour is produced. An actual example will make this clearer. A white rat is mated with a wild (brown or *gi'ey') rat, and since colour dominates over its absence the F^ heterozygotes are all grey, like wild rats. These gi'ey heterozygotes mated together give

grey x white

grey x grey

3 grey

\ white GGpp

X black

BBPP

grey

GBPp

X grey

GBPp

9 grey

1GGPP 2GB PP 2GG Pp 4 GBPp

3 black

1 BBPP 2BBP0

4- white

7GG pp 2 GB pp IBB pp

F4

coloured and albino in the ratio of three to one. If now one of these extracted albinos is mated with a black rat, the offspring may not be black but grey, and such grey individuals paired together will give young in the ratio of 9 grey, 3 black, 4 white.

The explanation is as follows. For the production of colour, two factors must be present, one for the

VI] MENDELIAN HEREDITY 73

production of pigment in general (P) and the other for the determination of the actual colour of that pigment (6^ = grey, J5 = black). Neither G nor B can produce any visible effect in the absence of P ; a rat without P (represented by p) is thus an albino. The extracted albinos in Pg from the cross wild grey X albino then contain G derived from their wild grandparent. These mated with black give grep offspring because grey is dominant^ over black, and the black individual introduces the factor P which was absent in the albino. These grey rats (generation Fs in the diagram) are thus heterozygous in the pair of factors grey and black (G and B) and in the factors presence and absence of P (P and p). They will thus produce gametes GP, Gp, BP, Bp, which in meeting at random will give 9 zygotes containing G and P, 3 containing B and P, 3 containing G and p, 1 con- taining B and p. But the combinations Gp and Bp, not having P, are albinos, and so we get 9 greys, 3 blacks, 4 whites.

If in the example just given nothing were known of the origin of the white rat which was crossed with the black (in the generation marked F^), it would be said that a white variety crossed with a black had

1 This explanation has been simplified by the omission of the fact that G and B do not represent factors for separate pigments, but that G consists in the addition of a pigment to hairs already containing B. A character dominant in this way is called ' epistatic,' see below p. 75.

74 HEREDITY [ch.

produced ' reversion on crossing ' and tlie young had reverted to the ancestral wild form. It is not of course necessary that the albino used to produce such a 'reversion' should itself be the offspring of a grey ; such grey-bearing albinos may be bred together for an indefinite number of generations, and still carry the factor (? ; or if they were ori- ginally derived from a black stock they would bear the factor B. When such stocks are crossed together heterozygous GB albinos are produced, and G and B segregate from one another in the albino just as in the coloured rats in which the colour-factor P is present. The fact that colour in animals and plants depends on the concurrent action of distinct factors thus explains the phenomena of 'reversion on crossing' which have so long been a puzzle to biologists.

Among the varieties of the brown (grey) rat only two colour types occur, grey (wild-colour) and black, but in the rabbit, mouse and other animals more are found. In the mouse there are four funda- mental colour-types, yellow, grey, black and chocolate. The behaviour of yellow is complicated and not yet thoroughly understood, but of the others, grey crossed with either black or chocolate gives grey ; black with chocolate gives black, and chocolate can only appear in the absence of all the others. This was formerly described by saying that grey was dominant over black and chocolate, and black over chocolate, but

VI] MENDELIAN HEREDITY 75

this is inconsistent with the hypothesis that allelo- morphs exist always in pairs, one possessing a factor lacking in the other. More correctly, then, each colour is allelomorphic with its absence, but the presence of a higher member of the series obscures or prevents the development of the lower. This is expressed by saying that grey is 'epistatic' over black and chocolate, and black over chocolate. Since chocolate is the lowest member of the series, it has been suggested that its factor is indeed the pigment factor represented in the case of the rats described above by the symbol P, and that in other colours the special factors are present in addition \ In grey mice yellow, black and chocolate pigments are all present in the hairs, but the factor for 'greyness' causes the yellow to be restricted to certain parts of the hair. In black mice both black and chocolate pigments are present, but the black obscures the chocolate, and in chocolate mice this pigment alone is present.

The object of this rather special digression is to show how the hypothesis of a series of colour-factors acting together can completely coordinate the pheno- mena of colour-inheritance, which very few years ago seemed hopelessly confused and subject to no definite rules. It is now possible to forecast with accuracy the results of a pairing between individuals of diflPerent

^ More recently a still lower member of the series, producing ' orange ' in the absence of the other colour-factors, has been described hy Hagedoorn.

0

76 HEREDITY [ch.

colours, if the constitution of the parents with respect to the colour-factors carried by them is known. Some of these cases have been exceedingly difficult to elu- cidate because it is often impossible by inspection to determine the constitution of a given individual. This must be tested by suitable matings with individuals of colour lower in the series, and it is then found that the results observed agree closely with expectation.

A more surprising instance of 'reversion on crossing' was discovered by Bateson in sweet-peas. He found that within the white variety known as * Emily Henderson ' two distinct types exist, indis- tinguishable in appearance, which when crossed together give a purple closely resembling the wild sweet-pea of Southern Europe. The purple rever- sionary form in the first cross, (Fi), self-fertilised, gives in the next generation, (F2), 9 coloured to 7 whites. The explanation is that some plants of the white form lack one colour factor (called by Bateson 'C'); others lack the complementary factor 'R,' which if present with C, would produce red pigment. Since colour can only appear when both O and R are present, each parental form is white, but when crossed together O and R are combined in one plant and coloured flowers result. The allelomorphic pairs are C and its absence (c), and R and its absence (r) ; the purple heterozygote is thus CcRr, and produces four kinds of gametes CR, Cr, cR, cr. These mating at

VI] MENDELIAN HEREDITY H

random give offspring in the ratio of 9 with C and /?, 3 with c and R, 3 with G and r, 1 with c and r. But only those containing both G and R can produce colour and therefore 9 coloured appear to 7 white. Further, among the coloured individuals of F<i, both purple and red appear, because the factors G and R together produce only red ; to get purple a third factor for blue (5) must also be present, which can only take effect in the presence of both G and R. Since B was introduced by one only of the original whites, the i^i purples were heterozygous for blue as well as for G and R (with fully represented constitution GcRrBh) and hence among the Fo plants one quarter contain no B and in the presence of G and R are red\

1 In this account, the production of colour (red) is described as being due to two factors (C and R). The recent work of Miss Wheldale [42] on the chemical nature of flower-colours indicates that the essential bodies are an organic base or ' chromogen ' and an oxidising ferment. The work of Chodat and Bach, however, indicates that such oxidising ferments must contain two components, neither of which alone is able to oxidise the chromogen and produce the coloured derivative anthocyanin. Both kinds of white sweet- pea contain the chromogen, but it seems probable that one component of the oxidising ferment is present only in one, and the other com- ponent only in the other. Hence no colour can be produced in either. But on mating the two whites together, the mechanism for the oxidation of the chromogen is again complete, and red colour (anthocyanin) is formed. The purple colour (represented by the additional ' factor ' B) is due to a further stage of the oxidation of the chromogen than when only red is produced. In some white

78 HEREDITY [CH.

In the account given above of the colour-factors in the sweet-pea it has been shown that at least two separate elements are required to produce colour (in this case red), and a third if blue is to be present in addition. But for the production of the various shades or distribution of colour further factors are known, e.g. for the intensification or dilution of colour, and for making the wing-petals of the same or difierent colour from the standard. Similar phenomena are concerned with colour in animals, of which domestic varieties of the rat provide a simple instance. Rats, other than albinos, are in general either 'self-coloured,' with little or no white (this, if present, is confined to the ventral surface), or 'hooded,' i.e. white with

flowers (snapdragon) experiment shows that the chromogen itself may be absent.

As the purple colour in sweet-peas is due to more complete oxidation of a chromogen than red, so in animals colour-physiologists find that the series yellow, brown, black, may represent successive oxidation-stages of the same chromogen by the same ferment. The various colours of mice, for instance, are not therefore to be regarded as necessarily produced by different ferments, but the inherited 'colour-factors' determine to what stage the oxidation of the chromogen shall be carried. Some confusion has arisen from the assumption that the ' factors ' postulated by students of heredity are actual specific colour-ferments, while they may be rather determinants which cause the oxidation of the chromogen to proceed to a particular stage, and may be compared with the factors which determine the production of a rose-comb or single- comb in fowls.

VI] MENDELIAN HEREDITY 79

coloured head and shoulders and a coloured stripe along the spine. The self and hooded factors are an allelomorphic pair independent of colour, so that a hooded rat may be black or grey. The factors may also be borne by albinos, and when very young an albino bearing the factor for the hood may be dis- tinguished by the different texture of the hair on the head and shoulders, giving the appearance of a water-mark or 'ghost-hood.' The heterozygote between self-coloured and hooded patterns differs from either parent, being black above and white below the so-called ' Irish ' type of the fancy. Such * Irish ' rats bred together always give both self-coloured and hooded rats in addition to Irish among their progeny. A similar case in rabbits is that of the well-known Dutch marking, which seems to correspond with the hooded condition in rats. In flowers the number of such characters determining the nature and distribu- tion of colour may be considerable, so that among the offspring of a cross between two varieties of Chinese primulas or snapdi-agons a very large colour-series may be produced, which on first in- spection may seem a continuous series from the darkest to the palest ; but careful analysis of these cases has shown that the different factors may be recognised and isolated, and the series of colours falls strictly within the rules of Mendelian inherit- ance when each factor is considered alone.

80 HEREDITY [ch.

Hitherto in discussing the interaction of distinct pairs of factors (allelomorphs), colour alone has been considered, but cases are known where colour and a structural character are interdependent in the same way. Interesting examples of this are known in stocks and primulas. When a certain smooth- leaved cream-flowered stock is crossed with a smooth white, the F^ plants are purple and hoary, i.e. they revert to the ancestral wild purple and hoary-leaved stock. The purple colour appears for the same reason that the two forms of white sweet-pea gave purple ; one colour-factor is introduced by the white parent and its complement by the cream \ But the hoari- ness appears because the parents contain a factor for hoariness, which can only take efiect in plants with purple, flowers. The parents are therefore smooth although they contain the hoary factor. When the Fx hoary purples are crossed together, the F^ genera- tion consists of purple, white and cream-flowered plants in the expected proportions, but only the purples are hoary. Smooth-leaved purple strains do exist, but these are plants lacking the hoary factor altogether ; if it were present, it would appear when- ever the flowers contain purple sap.

^ The cream-colour is due to a quite distinct factor, and the pigment is in special bodies (plastids) in the cells of the petals. The purple colour is due to a pigment dissolved in the sap, and is independent of the cream plastid-colour.

VI]

MENDELIAN HEREDITY

81

B

Fig. 10. Sections of Chinese Primula Flowers,

A. Long-style (' pin-eye ').

B. Short-style ('thrum-eye').

C. Homostyle.

I>.

6

82 HEREDITY [ch.

In Chinese Primulas a curious case of inter- relation between flower-colour and structure has been investigated by Bateson and Gregory. They find that the long-styled and short-styled types of flowers, so well known from Darwin's work, are an allelomorphic pair, short-styled being dominant. But when the long-styled factor is associated with a con- dition in which the yellow eye of the flower is enlarged to cover about half the area of the petals, the style remains short, although the anthers occupy the typical long-styled position in the tube of the flower. This con- dition is called 'homostyle' (fig. 10 C, p. 81). When a short-styled small-eyed plant is crossed with homostyle large-eyed, all the (Ft) ofi*spring are short-styled and small-eyed, these characters being dominant. But in the second generation, (F2), obtained by breeding to- gether these Fi plants, the following types appear :

9 short-style with small eye, 3 short-style with large eye, 3 long-style with small eye, 1 homostyle with large eye.

The long-styled form has appeared in Fo_ from short- style X homostyle, because homostyle is a condition of long-style modified by association with the large eye. When this association is broken, the long-style appears.

From these examples of the interaction of distinct

VI]

MENDELIAN HEREDITY

83

allelomorpliic pairs, many more of which are now known, it will be seen that many of the * exceptions ' to the Mendelian rule which have been recorded may

Fig. 11. Some of the types of flowers in generation from the cross short-style (thrum) small eye x homostyle large eye.

A. Long-style, small eye. B. Homostyle, large eye. C. Short- style, small eye. D. Short-style, large eye.

In A and B the flowers are of the ' star ' type. This character is inherited independently of the style and ' eye ' characters. (From Bateson.)

R— 2

84 HEREDITY [ch. vi

be explicable on the assumption that what appears to be a simple character is really dependent on two or more distinct factors, which become separated on crossing with a different form.

In conclusion, it must be mentioned that a number of cases are now known in which a pair of Mendelian characters are closely associated with Sex. In some cases the sex of the individual determines whether a character is dominant or recessive ; for example, if a horned race of sheep is crossed with a hornless, the male offspring are horned and the females hornless ; and in cats, it appears that yellow crossed with black gives yellow males but tortoiseshell females. In other cases certain Mendelian characters can be borne only by germ-cells which will give rise to one or the other sex. This aspect of the subject, which includes some of the most interesting recent advances in our know- ledge of Heredity, will be considered in a subsequent chapter (ix), after some other questions in which the sex-factor is not directly concerned have been dis- cussed.

CHAPTER VII

SOME DISPUTED QUESTIONS

In this chapter will be briefly considered certain questions which either are still quite unsettled, or upon which there is stiU active disagreement among biologists. It will be convenient to take first some which are closely connected with the Mendelian theory of heredity, and pass on later to others which are related equally to any theory of inheritance which may be adopted.

One of the chief lines of attack on the Mendelian theory has been the proposition that the absolutely complete segregation of allelomorphic characters in the germ-cells, postulated by that theory, has not been proved. If the theory is rigidly true, then in the case of a tall pea crossed with a short (Chap, v) the homozygous tails and shorts among the ofispring of the cross should be pure for tallness or short- ness as the original parents ; neither character should

86 HEREDITY [ch.

have been influenced in any way by its association with the other. It has been maintained that the Mendelian categories are not sufficiently definite to allow such a statement to be made with certainty. The Mendelian can only reply, that in the great majority of cases the ' extracted ' pure individuals in the F2 generation do not differ recognisably from the original parents in the characters considered, and that no signs of impurity can be found in later generations.

There are however instances in which it appears that Mendelian segregation may not be perfect. It has been maintained that an instance of this is provided by hair-length in guinea-pigs. When a long-haired ('Angora') guinea-pig is mated with a short-haired, the F^ offspring are short-haired, shortness being dominant, owing perhaps to the presence of a factor which prevents the growth of the hair after reaching a certain length. But when such Fi (heterozygous) short-hairs were mated together, in addition to apparently pure longs and shorts, animals with hair of intermediate length were produced, and these crossed back with pure long-hairs gave no short-haired young. It is suggested that the long and short characters have become fused in some germ-cells, segregation being incomplete or non-existent, so that germ-cells bearing the mixed character are produced. Again, in a cross between

VII] DISPUTED QUESTIONS 87

lop-eared and short-eared rabbits, young with ears of intermediate length are produced, and these mated together give no evidence of segregation in the next generation. From these and some other similar observations it must be concluded, either that in some cases there is incomplete segregation or even complete fusion of alternative characters, or that what appear to be simple characters are really com- plex, and that the true-breeding intermediates are formed by a new combination of elementary factors. An instance, which is perhaps similar, will be men- tioned in the next chapter in discussing the inheritance of pigment in Man.

A second question with regard to Mendelian segregation has at present received hardly any answer, namely, whether the apparently 'continuous' variations such as were illustrated by the example of Johannsen's beans (Chap, iv) segregate according to the Mendelian rule. It was seen that each 'pure line,' derived by self-fertilisation from an individual plant, has its own type about which the size of the beans borne by the plant fluctuates ; but it is not known whether, when two plants having different types (modal sizes) are crossed, segregation takes place between the two types in the formation of the germ- cells of the crossed individual. To determine this would naturally require a long and difficult series of experiments, and at present very little is known of

88 HEREDITY [ch.

the subject, but when it is remembered how wide- spread and in what varied characters segregation has been found, it would not seem improbable that it should occur in such cases also.

Another subject which Mendelian investigation has brought into prominent notice, and which has led to much controversy, is the kind of variation which has been effective in the process of evolution. Darwin assumed that evolution takes place by the preservation of very small ' continuous ' variations which occur in a direction favourable to the species, but even among his immediate followers, for example Huxley, doubt was expressed whether larger step-like variations or 'mutations' may not have been operative. Darwin rejected this idea chiefly on the ground of the rarity of such mutations, which makes it inevitable that the mutating individual should generally mate with one of the normal type, and so it was supposed that the mutation would be diluted and rapidly lost. But Mendelian work shows that this dilution does not occur in simple cases ; the offspring of the cross between the mutation and the type produces half its germ-cells bearing the mutation to its full extent, and these will transmit the mutation until the race may become widely infected with it, and not infre- quently individuals both of which possess it will mate together. If the mutation be dominant, as in the case of the well-known black variety of the 'Peppered

VII] DISPUTED QUESTIONS 89

Moth' (Amphidasys hetvlaria), it may spread rapidly until it becomes common, and if recessive it will equally often be represented in the germ-cells of many individuals and will appear when two which bear it mate together. In either case if the mutation be advantageous it may be preserved at the expense of the type by natural selection, until it obtains a firm footing. But the difficulty has naturally been felt that the marvellously perfect adaptations which are so frequent in nature cannot be imagined to have arisen by large steps, but must have been ac- quired gradually, and therefore many naturalists reject the suggestion that mutations can have been largely operative in evolution. The fallacy here is the assumption that all discontinuous variations must be large ; the case of Johannsen's beans shows that essentially stable variations occur, which pro- bably differ from mutations only in their small extent, and by the selection of such 'minute mutations' the wonderfully perfect structures of living things might be produced. It may perhaps be regarded as hair-splitting to distinguish between minute mutations and fluctuating variability, but the distinction lies in the nature of their inheritance, which is the essential thing in evolution. It has been seen that no selection within the 'pure line' in the case of the beans has any effect ; for progress to be made, a new mutation, small though it might be, is necessary.

90 HEREDITY [CH.

The problem in heredity which has probably given rise to more controversy than any other is that alluded to more than once previously, of the inherit- ance or non-inheritance of acquired characters, that is, characters produced in the individual during its life by the action of some sort of stimulus. Some aspects of the question have already been considered, and from what has been shown of the very definite nature of the inheritance of germinal (inborn) cha- racters, it will be understood why students of heredity are increasingly disposed, a priori, to disbelieve in the transmission of acquirements ; for if these were transmitted to any considerable extent, this fact must interfere, one would suppose, with the orderly appearance in the offspring of the characters repre- sented in the germ-cells of the parents. But at the present time no treatment of heredity could be re- garded as complete without some mention of the evi- dence which has been adduced in favour of the transmission of such characters. Unfortunately, the evidence is almost always capable of interpretation in more than one sense. The supporters of the belief in transmission rely largely on indirect evidence, especially on the difficulty of imagining any cause of evolution in certain directions if the eflfects of acquirement are excluded. A vast literature has grown up around this question, of which only illustrative examples can be given. In animals which

VII] DISPUTED QUESTIONS 91

live in the dark the pigment in the skin is frequently absent, as it is also in flat-fish on the side of the body which lies protected from light on the sea-floor. It is said that pigment in such cases cannot be harmful, and so its disappearance is not due to natural selection. But pigment very generally appears in response to the action of light, and so it is supposed that the absence of the stimulus to production, acting for many generations, has caused the pigment to dis- appear. This is illustrated by the well-known experiment of Cunningham on flat-fish. The young fish is pigmented on both sides of the body ; it then settles on one side and the pigment on that side disappears. Cunningham reared such young fish in an aquarium lighted from below : when they settled on the bottom the pigment disappeared, but if kept still longer exposed to light from beneath, the pigment began to come back again. The dis- appearance of the pigment, although exposed to light, proves that the loss is hereditary ; its return on continued exposure to light is interpreted by Cunningham to mean that its disappearance was due to absence of light, and has gradually become hereditary, but that the process can be reversed by again exposing the lower side to the action of the stimulus. The same argument has been used with regard to the colourless skins and vestigial eyes of animals living in caves ; where the structure is use-

92 HEREDITY [ch.

less it disappears, and the most obvious cause to assume is lack of use, which, acting cumulatively through many generations, has become hereditary. Such evidence, however, is only presumptive, it does not amount to proof, and on the other side may be adduced the pigments of birds' eggs. Birds which nest in holes or dark places usually have colourless or slightly coloured eggs, while those which lay in open places have eggs more or less matching their surroundings. This appears closely comparable with the condition of skin-colour in fishes and amphibians, and yet it is impossible that the action of light could have any direct efiect on the production of pigment in birds' eggs. That the loss of pigment in each case is connected with its uselessness is probable, but the birds' egg case seems to show that it is not due to * use-inheritance.'

A second instance of the indirect evidence for the inheritance of acquired characters may be given, that of instinct. Instincts are very similar to firmly rooted habits, and have been regarded as habits which from being performed through many generations have become hereditary. There can be no doubt that, in the higher animals especially, instincts may be rein- forced and perfected by habit, but many cases can be adduced in which it seems impossible that habit has played a part in the evolution of an instinct. Many insects have exceedingly perfect and complex in-

viij DISPUTED QUESTIONS 93

stincts in connexion with egg-laying, yet the process may last only a few hours, and the eggs may all be fully formed and ready for laying before the insect hatches from the pupa. In the worker-bee, too, there are many admirably developed instincts, and also structural features which might be thought to have originated by the transmission of acquired adaptations, and yet the worker-bee, except in rare cases, never repro- duces itself, but is produced by a queen and a drone with structures and instincts diiFerent from its own. If in these cases we find perfect instincts which cannot have arisen by the inheritance of acquire- ments, it seems unreasonable to assume that instincts in other species must have arisen in this way. These two cases are given merely as examples of the pre- sumptive evidence that has been brought forward- It is admitted that the process of evolution would be more easily comprehensible if the inheritance of acquired characters were a fact, but it is clear that no absolute proof of its existence can be based on cases of this kind.

Exact experiments on the possible inherited efiects of acquirements are difficult to devise so as to be unequivocal, and most have given negative results \ A case which at first sight seems to prove

1 Experiments on moths and butterflies have been mentioned in Chapter in ; in Bome, notably those of Fischer with the Tiger Moth,

94 HEREDITY [ch.

the inherited eiFects of conditions is the experiment of Kellogg in starving silkworms, in which he found that when the caterpillars were starved for two generations, the third generation, even if well fed, were below the normal size. But there is here a possible source of error, that the eggs produced by starved females may have been lacking in yolk, so that the resulting caterpillars would be weakly from the beginning and never overtake the normal size. If so, the apparent effect of inheritance of bad conditions would be due really to poor embryonic nourishment, not to germinal difference. The same explanation might apply to the apparent cumulative effects of under-feeding in man, if the mother cannot adequately nourish the infant before birth. The famous experiments of Brown-Sequard on the in- heritance of artificial injuries in guinea-pigs must be mentioned. He found that when the parents were subjected to operations of various kinds, some of the young showed corresponding abnormalities, especially in the case of the effects of certain injuries to the nervous system. Subsequent experiments however have not completely confirmed his results, and there is reason to believe that where they have

Arctia cnja, definite evidence for the transmission of modifications was obtained, but this may have been due to direct modification of the germ-cells themselves.

VII] DISPUTED QUESTIONS 95

been confirmed there are other possible explanations of the apparent transmission of the effects of injury. For example, Brown-Sequard found that when the chief nerve of the leg is severed, the toes become morbid and the animals frequently nibble them away. A small percentage of the offspring of guinea-pigs lacking toes from this cause also had toes missing. But it has been pointed out that rodents in captivity sometimes eat off the toes or tails of their young, and if the mother had acquired the habit of nibbling her own toes, she might bite off those of her young shortly after birth and give the appearance of the inheritance of a mutilation [20, 29]. Evidence has also been brought forward that the mutilation of the parents may cause the production of a toxin, which is transmitted directly to the offspring, and causes abnormalities to appear in them in the same organs that were injured in the parent.

Quite recently Kammerer [19] in Vienna has made some remarkable observations on salamanders and a species of toad which seem to support the idea of the inheritance of acquired characters. For example, among other experiments, he finds that the animals can be accustomed to lay their eggs in water instead of on land, and tlie young become modified to suit their new surroundings, and the modifications are progressively increased in later generations. He points out however that most of his results, like

96 HEREDITY [ch.

those obtained by Tower (Chap. Ill), may have been brought about by the action of environment on the eggs at the time of maturation, but they differ from Tower's in the regularity of their appearance and in being adaptive. Further work in this direction will be awaited with interests

Keference must also be made to the work of Sumner [32 a] who finds that rats kept at a high temperature differ in several particulars (proportions of the body, etc.) from those kept in a cold room. These characteristics are inherited to a measurable extent, and the ex- planation offered in the case of insects, that the germ-cells were directly affected by the temperature, is not easy to apply to a warm- blooded animal. There may of course be indirect effects, brought about by a change in general constitution, but the admission of this hardly differs from accepting the inheritance of acquired characters. A considerable list of cases of the transmission of environmental modification, chiefly but not entirely in plants, is given by M^Dougall [22 a]. An important contribution to the subject is the recently published work of Boas, reviewed in Nature, Nov. 3, 1910, p. 11. He finds from a study of the children of European immigrants into New York that whatever the racial characters of the parents, the children born after their arrival in America tend to approach a certain type, and approach it more nearly, the longer the parents have lived in New York. For example, the children of a long-headed race have progressively shorter and rounder heads, the longer the parents have lived in America before their children are born, while those of round- headed parents become longer so as to approach the same type. In this case there is no question of the inheritance of 'acquirements' in the ordinary sense, for the shape of the parents' heads is not affected ; it appears that either the germ-cells or the young childien themselves are affected in such a way as to cause them to approach a characteristic American type in head-shape and other characters.

VII] DISPUTED QUESTIONS 97

On the whole, the hypothesis of the inheritance of acquired characters must be regarded as 'not proven/ and our increasing knowledge of the definite- ness of many germinal characters makes it doubtful whether it can be a factor of great importance in the constitution of the individual, or to the course of evolution. Some further evidence in this direction will be given in the next chapter \

A few minor questions remain. One of these, which has played a considerable part in biological literature, is the alleged phenomenon called Telegony.

^ The recent evidence which has been brought forward on the subject warns us against a dogmatic denial of the possibility of the inheritance of acquired modifications. The number of cases recorded is now considerable, in which adaption to changed environment, either of structure or instinct, appears to be transmitted to the next generation. The tendency of biological thought is certainly towards a recognition of the unity of the organism as a whole, including its germ-cells, and especially where the organism adapts itself to change, it seems possible that this adaptation is transmissible. The belief that 'somatic' changes could not be transmitted rests largely on the idea that every character is determined by a 'factor' or determinant in the germ-cell, but it is clear that any character is not developed directly from the germinal determinant, but by the relation existing between the determinant and its surroundings, viz. the body of the organism. If the surroundings are changed, this relation may be altered, and the altered relation may be transmitted to the off- spring, so bringing about a corresponding change in the character as it appears in the next generation. How far such changes are of real importance, and whether they are permanent or, so to speak, temporary expedients to meet changed conditions, is still an open question. Students of heredity generally would take the latter view.

D. 7

98 HEREDITY [CH.

It was formerly believed, and the belief is still firmly held by fanciers and animal breeders, that if a female of one breed bears young by a male of another breed, and is then mated with a male of her own kind, the ofispring of this second mating will in some cases show the influence of the first sire, and instead of being pure-bred will in some respects be mongrels resembling the mongrel offspring of the first mating. The instance of this made classical by Darwin is 'Lord Morton's Mare,' in which a chestnut mare bore a colt by a quagga, and afterwards two colts by a black Arab stallion, both of which were dun-coloured, and bore stripes on the legs and in one colt on the neck also [7]. But it is known that dun horses are frequently striped to some extent, and Ewart's well-known work with zebras [11], in which it was attempted to repeat this experiment, gave negative results. The belief in telegony is widely held among dog-fanciers, and many cases could be quoted, but whenever properly controlled experiments have been made, no evidence of telegony has been forthcoming. The belief in it is almost certainly due to the habit of generalising from individual instances ; whenever a case occurs which appears to favour the belief, it is adduced as proof, even though other causes may have been operative, and matings in which no evidence for it appears are passed over in silence. If it were a genuine phenomenon, it is almost certain that

VII] DISPUTED QUESTIONS 99

conclusive evidence for it would have been obtained in the numerous breeding experiments recorded in recent years.

Another idea very widely held, but apparently resting on no better evidence, is the belief in maternal impressions, especially in the case of mankind. By maternal impression is meant the influencing of the child by events affecting the mother during pregnancy. It is commonly believed that if a pregnant woman is injured in any part, or even sees an injury to another person so as to excite her imagination, the corresponding part in the child may be abnormally developed, or may bear some mark, caused, it is supposed, by an impression conveyed from the mother. More general still is the belief that the temperament of the child is influenced by the mother's mental condition during pregnancy. This latter belief is scarcely susceptible of accurate investigation, but the belief in bodily marks or malformations being due to corresponding injury to the mother, or to her attention being strongly attracted to that part, is almost certainly based on coincidence. A large number of children are born with some abnormality, and a very large proportion have some mark on the skin. Many mothers during pregnancy undergo some slight accident or see some deformed person, and thus it must happen that a mark on the child will often

7—2

100 HEREDITY [CH. vii

roughly coincide in position with the part affected in the parent. If every coincidence of this kind is quoted as proof of the reality of maternal impression, and the cases are left unheeded in which no relation can be found between abnormality in the child and events affecting the mother, it is natural that a belief in the phenomenon will easily take firm root. The evidence available however is probably insuffi- cient to support any other view than that of accidental coincidence.

CHAPTER VIII

HEREDITY IN MAN

In the chapters dealing with the various aspects of heredity in general, a number of instances have been given illustrating inheritance of various cha- racters in man, and the province of this chaptei* will be to collect and add to these cases, so as to sketch the general outlines of what is known of human inheritance. It has been seen that as man differs in no important way in his bodily characters from the other mammalia, so the laws governing the variation and transmission of those characters are the same as are found throughout the animal and vegetable kingdoms wherever they have been in- vestigated ; and further that the 'mental and moral' attributes of man, which presumably are correlated with physical structures, are inherited no less strongly than the bodily features themselves. When investi- gated by the biometric methods, the stature, span, length of fore-arm, eye-colour, and certain other physical characters or measurements, are found to

102 HEREDITY [ch.

give a parental correlation and thus an intensity of inheritance closely similar to those obtained from the study of various animals and plants. When various non-measurable and less definite characters such as intellectual ability, hand-writing, etc. are investigated by the same methods, a similar intensity of heredity is found, and finally the same is true when the character chosen is liability to certain diseases, notably tuberculosis and insanity, or such abnormal conditions as congenital deafness. Since these latter conditions have been only briefly alluded to, and are of such fundamental importance for the well-being of mankind, the evidence may be referred to rather more fully here. The case of insanity is especially convincing, for it is not open to the objection some- times made with regard to infectious diseases that the cause of the apparent inheritance of the condition is the exposure of the child to infection from the parent. It must be remembered that there are many kinds of insanity, in one of which at least (chorea), the inheritance appears to be Mendelian, and that of two men with equal tendency to mental aberration, one who is not exposed to strain may remain normal through life, while another under more arduous conditions may break down. But the data of occurrence of insanity among tainted stocks make it certain that ' the insane diathesis is inherited with at least as great an intensity as any physical or

VIII] HEREDITY IN MAN 103

mental character in man. It forms... probably no exception to an orderly system of inheritance in man, whereby on the average about one-half of the mean parental character, whether physical, mental or pathological, will be found in the child. It is accord- ingly highly probable that it is in the same manner as other physical characters capable of selection or elimination by unwise or prudential mating in the course of two or three generations.' (Heron [10]). Similarly for congenital deafness, Schuster writes '...that striking confirmation has been obtained of previous work on widely different characters, at any rate with regard to the correlation between father and children, and mother and children.' [31].

These examples, which might be added to, of results obtained by 'biometric' methods, make it sufficiently clear that a knowledge of the facts of inheritance is of importance to mankind, and that the further collection of accurate data is one of the most needed social requirements. Before passing on to other aspects of the question one other subject may be mentioned. The measure of resemblance in these characters has not only been worked out between parent and child, but between brothers and sisters, between children and grandparents and uncles and aunts, and between cousins. Some esti- mate can therefore be made of the probability of an individual being afiected if his relatives are known,

104 HEREDITY [ch.

a thing which should not only be useful to insurance offices, but to all thinking men, for it may ultimately become the basis for deciding on the propriety of marriage by members of tainted families. In general it appears that the resemblance of a child to its grand- parent is rather more than half of that to its parent, and that the resemblance between uncle and nephew, or between first cousins, is very slightly less than between grandchild and grandparent.

We may now turn to definitely discontinuous cha- racters in man, some of which are clearly Mendelian in their inheritance. One of the most interesting cases is that of eye-colour. Hurst [17] has shown that complete absence of pigment in the front of the iris is recessive to the presence of pigment ; that is to say, that two pure blue-eyed people have only blue- eyed offspring, but that a blue-eyed individual married to one with any brown or yellow in the iris may have children with pigmented eyes and that two pigmented parents have pigmented children, with or without a proportion of blue-eyed in addition. Within the pigmented class there is great range of variation, from a small yellow rim round the pupil to com- pletely dark eyes, and the relation of the various pigmented types to each other has not yet been analysed. But that the characters ' pigmented ' and ' non-pigmented ' are a Mendelian pair, his evidence leaves no doubt. This is thus a case in which the

VIII] HEREDITY IN MAN 105

occurrence of apparently continuous variation within a discontinuous category is clearly shown. Of other Mendel ian characters in man, colour-blindness, com- plicated by its relation Avith sex, has already been

Fig. 12. Bracbydactylous bands. (From Bateson, after Farabee.)

mentioned and is further considered in Chapter ix. Several cases are known in which an abnormality behaves as a simple dominant, e.g. the condition of

106 HEREDITY [ch.

the fingers known as 'brachydactyly,' in which the fingers have one joint less than the normal ; congenital cataract, and probably other diseases of the eye. Perhaps the most remarkable human pedigree ever collected is one of 'night-blindness/ extending through nine generations and going back to the seventeenth century, which has been published by Nettleship (see [1]). The condition is one in which the patient cannot see in dull light, and it behaves as a Mendelian dominant, probably, however, with some complication, since the numbers aflected are less than the theore- tical expectation. In all these cases in which the abnormality is dominant, only affected individuals can transmit it ; the normal members of the family have only normal ofi*spring, a condition which is shortly summarised as 'once free, always free.'

The rule that the affected alone transmits will be followed only when the condition depends on a single factor ; if it depends on more than one, or if its dominance is modified by sex or other conditions, then non-afiiected individuals may have affected ofi- spring. This is possibly the case in many diseases in which it appears that the affection is dominant, and yet certain non-affected individuals have affected offspring, and in such examples it must also be remembered that the disease is probably not always developed in people in whom the tendency is present ; the tendency may be there but the conditions required

VIIl]

HEREDITY IN JNIAN

107

f <^ rfcT^^I^^® (ifcc|3'iro«|6 ® ^2...®?...$ ..^f!".!?.*!.^ ®

IX\ /AX,'

J

I I I J I Ml I .^ _^ .

I

^ iM^

T I I I I 1 n

I I r I / I I I I V 1 rn r-i r^ r*-!

OiC9909 cTd" 9 9 5 9cf^9ia®f 90

1 /J^A^|^\^

I @ ®

€>

I I I I I

cf Unaffected male 9 Unaffected female

^ Affected male t Affected female

I Not certainly affected 9 ^

f 3 females

® 3 individuals, sex not recorded

Indicates consanguinity.

108 HEREDITY [CH.

to bring out the disease may be avoided, especially if it is a condition not present at birth, but appearing later in life. This kind of thing may perhaps be illustrated by the pedigrees of Retinitis pigmentosa taken from Nettleship (Bowman Lecture, 1909 [23]) on p. 105.

The disease is not usually present at birth, but comes on at a varying age, sometimes during or after middle life, and it will be seen that in the first pedigree it is transmitted only by affected members of the family, so behaving as a typical Mendelian dominant. In the second pedigree, however, it com- monly 'skips' a generation, the parents of affected individuals usually being normal, but themselves children or sisters of those who are affected. The fact that in both families (as in most cases of this disease) males are more frequently affected than females, suggests that there is some complication, and this is perhaps connected with the fact that in one family the disease behaves as a simple dominant, while in the other it is most frequently transmitted, like colour-blindness, through normal females from affected males. These pedigrees are given as examples of the somewhat irregular inheritance of diseased con- ditions such as may frequently be seen in the medical journals ; many of them are probably explicable in the ways suggested above.

A somewhat different group of phenomena is

VIII] HEREDITY IN MAN 109

illustrated by the iiilieritance of pigmentation in man, in skin- and hair-colour. In the case of hair- colour, Hurst has given evidence that bright red behaves as a recessive to dark-coloured hair, and that to some extent at least segregation takes place. But the shades of haii'-colour graduate into one another so continuously that it is impossible to place them with confidence in Mendelian categories, and further the colour alters so greatly between infancy and maturity in many persons that classifi- cation is difficult. Many children for example have bright red hair, in whom during adolescence the colour deepens to brown, while other members of the same family, whose hair has hardly diflered from the first during childhood, keep the bright red until middle life. In families with red hair we may see clear evidence of segregation between red and dark brown hair-colour, but the differences between the originally red-haired individuals show that some contain a darkening factor which is absent in the others. Probably then in human hair-colour there are a number of factors which interact upon one another in a way even more complex than in the hair-colour of mice mentioned in a previous chapter ; and where rigid experiment is impossible, the analysis of these factors is almost hopeless. The same remarks perhaps apply to the various eye-colours within the class with pigmented iris, and the very frequent but

no HEREDITY [ch.

not absolutely perfect correlation between dark eye- colour and dark hair suggests that similar factors may perhaps be at work in both cases \

The inheritance of skin-colour in man is also one of the cases which has hitherto defied Mendelian analysis, and has been quoted more than once as disproving the universality of Mendelian inheritance. When a * white ' European marries a negro, the ofi- spring are 'mulattoes/ intermediate between the parents. Mulattoes however are not all alike, some have brown skins and some yellowish. When they marry among themselves they are said never to produce full blacks or full whites, but again mulat- toes, who however vary in the depth of their colour. When a midatto marries a white, the 'quadroon' offspring are lighter than mulattoes but usually darker than Europeans ; there is evidence however that they vary considerably, with possibly a certain amount of discontinuity between the darkest and lightest. Some evidence of segregation is also pro- vided by the occasional instances of ' throw back * to very dark skin and negi'oid features or hair among children of apparently white people with some negro ancestry. The whole problem however is very insuffi- ciently known, and the difficulty of obtaining reliable

' In this connexion it may be mentioned that two recent writers have maintained that the famous 'Habsburg lip' and the peculiar Jewish physiognomy are Mendelian characters, the former dominant, the latter recessive to the normal type.

VIII] HEREDITY IN MAN 111

data is doubtless increased by race-prejudice. Taken in mass, the results of crossing white and black races seem to give a blended inheritance with continuous variation ; but as has been seen in the case of hair- colour the accurate investigation of individual families would possibly show that several factors were con- cerned, and that in the later generations, only when all these factors are combined in one individual would the colour be identical with that of either of the original races. In this respect crosses between different races of mankind resemble hybrids between different species of animals or plants, except that there is usually no sterility. Most of the Mendelian investigations have been made on varieties which differ in few characters, for the sake of simplicity, but when species are crossed and the offspring are fertile so many diverse characters are concerned, of which the relation to one another is not generally known, that the offspring of the hybrids may con- tain no individuals closely resembling either parent species. This has been explained by saying that only varietal and not specific characters segregate from one another on the Mendelian scheme, but it is not improbably due to the multiplicity of characters con- cerned, and their complicated interrelations, which makes analysis exceedingly difficult. It is also not impossible, when germ-cells differing very con- siderably in constitution combine in fertilisation, that in the formation of the germ-ceUs of the next

112 HEREDITY [ch.

generation the machinery for segregation is inade- quate. Extreme cases of this are possibly the cause of the frequent sterility of hybrids, but it may be that when the parental differences areinsufficient to prevent the formation of fertile germ-cells, they may yet be enough to interfere with normal Mendelian segregation. Certain aspects of inheritance in mankind have now been reviewed, and it remains briefly to indicate the lines on which our knowledge may be of practical importance. One of the things which is especially prominent when the evidence is considered as a whole is the exceeding definiteness or determinancy of the process of heredity. Given parents of certain constitution, it can be said with confidence that on the average a certain proportion of their offspring will have such and such characters. It matters not whether the character considered is regarded from the standpoint of the Biometrician or the Mendelian, both agree that what is present in the germ-cell will be present in the individual, and that external con- ditions as a rule play but a small part in determining its appearance. The Biometrician finds an average value for the intensity of inheritance, and shows that it is sensibly the same whether the character con- sidered is stature, eye-colour, ability, or tendency to congenital disease. When the character in question is a simple case of presence or absence, the Men- delian finds that it is present in a definite proportion of the children of affected parents, so that he can say

VIII] HEREDITY IN MAN 113

with confidence that among the offspring of a parent who has congenital cataract or abnormally jointed fingers, about one-half will be similarly affected, and there is no hope in such a case that the severity of the affection will diminish in later generations. Where the disease depends on several factors, it may perhaps be eliminated by repeated marriage with untainted stock, but in such cases as cataract or colour-blindness there is no hope of this.

It is commonly supposed that inherited disease arises largely from the cumulative effect of bad conditions, drink and the like, but it has been seen how doubtful it is whether the effects of such things are really transmitted, and in any case it can be proved that in comparison with the germinal con- stitution, the effects of environment are relatively insignificant. Galton was one of the first to illustrate this by the study of twins. Human twins are of two sorts ; in one case they arise by the simultaneous development of two ova, as in the litters of lower animals, and then they are no more alike than other children of the same parents, and may be of different sexes. Twins of the second kind are probably pro- duced by division of one ovum, and are then of the same sex and so alike as to be called 'identical.' Such identical' twins remain through life, despite differences of environment, more like one another than successively born brothers commonly are, even D. 8

114 HEREDITY [CH.

when brought up in precisely the same surroundings. The same thing has been shown by an investigation of school-children in relation to their home environment and the habits of their parents. From a study of over 70,000 children in Glasgow, classified according to the employment or non-employment of their mothers in work outside the home, it was found that the rela- tion of their height and weight to the employment or non-employment of the mother was almost negligible compared with the relation between the physical characters of the mother and child. Still more surprising, if correct, is the observation that no regular relation could be found between drinking habits in the parents and the health, intelligence or physical development of some 1400 children in the schools of Edinburgh. [Elderton, 10\] Investi- gations of this kind are still in their infancy, and perhaps more urgently needed than any other social data and it would be rash to make sweeping general statements from the little that has been done. Results like the examples quoted make one doubt

1 The conclusions arrived at from this investigation have been severely criticised from both the medical and the statistical side, and it is probable that the statistical material used is not capable of yielding a decisive answer to the question whether drinking habits in the parents cause deterioration of the children or not. Neverthe- less, the fact that it was not found suggests that the habits of the parents are relatively unimportant compared with the nature of the stock in determining the character of the children.

VIII] HEREDITY IN MAN 115

whether the generally accepted statements about the degeneration caused by unhealthy conditions or drink are really at all reliable. It is easy where insanity or other disease occurs, to say that in so many per cent, of the cases there has been alcoholism in the ancestors, and that therefore alcoholism is a cause of insanity ; but in the first place it must be shown that the alcoholism is not the result of nervous disorder, which in the next generation appears as insanity^ ; and in the second place, in order to prove a causal connexion, in addition to this it must be shown that insanity is actually more frequent in the descendants of drunkards than in those of the sober. The un- doubted evils of excessive drinking are many and obvious enough, but it does not follow that physical or mental degeneration of the descendants are among them, and it may be a false hope to suppose that these evils could be removed merely by the abolition of drink.

The same sort of argument may apply to the undoubted physical and mental inferiority of our slum population. It is not yet proved whether this is the effect of miserable surroundings, or whether the ' unfit ' gravitate to the worst places because the more fit occupy the better. These are problems which society has as yet scarcely attempted to face, and yet it is clear that on their correct solution

* In this connexion see Barrington and Pearson [10].

8—2

116 HEREDITY [ch.

depends the central question of social reform. If man is to any appreciable extent the creature of his environment, then improved conditions will improve the race. But if, as the study of heredity suggests, though it would be rash to say it is proved, man is almost entirely the product of inborn factors which are little aflFected by environment, then improved conditions may only encourage the pro- pagation of the degenerate, and the race as a whole may go back rather than forward. Respon- sible students are not lacking who maintain that this is already taking place. It is said that the increase of insanity which is believed to have taken place in modern times is due to the provision of asylums where the insane are properly cared for and fre- quently discharged as 'cured.' When the insane were treated on the ' strait jacket ' system no cure could be eflfected, and so the unfortunates could not recover to propagate their kind. But on the present system it not infrequently happens that the insane are enabled to bring into the world large families, so that it is not improbable that the increase in number may be due to this, rather than to the increased strain of modern conditions. No one would advocate a return to the old system, but some restriction on the reproduction of the mentally deficient is undoubtedly demanded by modern knowledge of heredity.

VIII] HEREDITY IN MAN 117

It is even said that hospitals and the feeding of destitute school-children are really working in the direction opposite to what is intended, by enabling the degenerate to live and beget families, who under harder conditions would never have survived \ If a child is to survive it is undoubtedly better that he should be well fed and cared for, but looking at the matter apart from all sentiment, it is quite possible that posterity will be worse rather than better as a result of such institutions. It is not improbable that future generations will find that our methods for the relief of distress are on wrong lines, and that other means must be found for dealing with the problem, which will cure the evil at its root instead of attempt- ing to alleviate the symptoms.

Another point at which the study of heredity touches social problems is the treatment of criminals. It is becoming recognised that a large proportion of criminals are in some way abnormal, and that their crimes are due not to evil surroundings nor to wilful perversity, but to inherited defects. If this is actually the case, penal treatment of such is no less cruel than similar treatment of the insane, but in both cases efforts at reclamation or cure, followed

^ It is of course not suggested that all or even the majority of those who receive such help are degenerate, but it can hardly be doubted that a very high proportion of the ' unfit ' will take advan- tage of it.

118 HEREDITY [ch. viii

by liberty and encouragement to marry, may only lead to a repetition of the same evils in the next generation. The present teaching of biology is per- fectly clear, that in the case of the evils mentioned above and many others, marriage of those afflicted, and to a less extent of their near relatives, involves a grave risk of transmitting the aflFection to descen- dants, and 80 of inflicting serious injury upon society^

1 See [43J.

CHAPTER IX

HEREDITY AND SEX

In all the cases discussed hitherto the characters considered have been transmitted independently of the sex of the parents or offspring. The results of reciprocal matings have been identical, and the distribution of the characters among the offspring has been independent of their sex. It is well known, however, that many characters, in animals especially, are confined to one sex or are developed differently in males and females; this is most con- spicuously so in 'secondary sexual characters,' that is to say, features characteristic of one sex only but not in any way directly connected with reproduction. As examples may be mentioned the distinctive plumage of many male birds, or patterns of butter- flies, the horns of male mammals, and the hair on the face in men. Darwin long ago showed that such distinctively male characters may be transmitted through the female, and it has recently been shown that for Pheasants the converse is also true. Before

120 HEREDITY [ch.

entering in greater detail into the discussion of such cases it will be simpler to consider some other instances of 'sex-limited' or sex-modified inheritance which have been worked out in recent years, and which will probably throw light on sexual dimorphism in general.

It has been seen that in Mendelian inheritance one allelomorphic character is commonly dominant over the other, but that many cases are known in which dominance is partial, and it is probable that in some instances a positive or 'present' character may be recessive in the sense that it cannot appear unless introduced into the individual (zygote) from both parents. Now recent work has shown that some characters are dominant in one sex but recessive or only partially visible in the other (when heterozygous). One of the first cases of this to be discovered was the Tortoiseshell and Yellow Cat, and although there are probably complications in this instance which are not fully worked out, yet it is fairly certain that in cats which are in constitution heterozygotes between yellow and black, in males the yellow is completely dominant, but in females the dominance is partial, giving tortoiseshell. That is to say, in yellow-black heterozygotes, in males the yellow colour completely suppresses the black, but in females the black appears with the yellow. Thus tortoiseshell cats are almost invariably females, the corresponding males being

IX] HEREDITY AND SEX 121

yellow. A more completely known case is that of horns in Sheep. If a sheep of a breed in whicli both sexes bear horns is mated with one of a completely hornless breed, among the F^ offspring the rams bear horns and the ewes are hornless. Such F^ young bred together give in F^ both horned and hornless males and females, and all the horned ewes and hornless rams are homozygous and breed true, while the horn- less ewes and horned rams may be either pure or heterozygous. Here we have a clear case of a character which is dominant in one sex (male) and recessive in the other (female).

In such a case as this it may be said that the character is transmitted independently of sex, but its appearance in heterozygous oifspring is modified or controlled by the sex of the individual. Both males and females receive the character alike, but it appears in males while it is suppressed in females of the same constitution.

In another important group of cases the actual distribution of the character among the offspring is determined by their sex, and a study of such throws important light upon the vexed question of sex-deter- mination and the nature of sex itself. As an instance of this we will first consider one of the best-known examples, that of the inheritance of the variety lacticolor in the common Currant Moth {Abraxas grossidariata). The very rare variety lacticolor is

122 HEREDITY [CH.

found wild almost solely in females. When mated with a typical (grossulariata) male all the offspring of both sexes are typical, i.e. the type is completely dominant over the variety lacticolor. When two such heterozygous individuals are paired together, lacticolor appears as would be expected in about one quarter of the offspring, but all the lacticolor si^ecimens are females. When a lacticolor female is mated with a heterozygous male, half the males and half the females

JFig. 13. Abraxas grossulariata and its var. lacticolor.

are lacticolor, that is to say, by this mating alone can the lacticolor character be transferred to the male sex. Lact. male x lact. female gives only lact. in both sexes, as would be expected from two recessives paired to- gether, but lact. male by heterozygous female gives all the females lact., all the males typical (heterozygous) grossulariata. Using the abbreviations L for variety lacticolor, G for the typical grossulariata character, G {L) for heterozygous grossulariata, ^ for male.

IX] HEREDITY AND SEX 123

and $ for female, these results may be summarised thus:

1.

L^xG $

gives gross. (^ , gross. $ .

2.

G{L)2^G{L)$

gives gross. $ , gross. 9 , lact. $ .

3.

L^xG{L) $

gives gross. ^,lact. $, gross. 2,lact. $

4.

G(L) ?xZ cJ

gives gross. $, I act. $.

5.

L <^xL S

gives lact. ^, lacL ?.

The important points about these facts are that lacticolor is clearly a recessive variety when crossed with grossidariata, but the distribution of the char- acter among- the sexes is dilJerent in the reciprocal crosses, nos. 3 and 4 above. The only explanation that has been offered to account for this is that females lay two kinds of eggs, one destined to become males and the other to become females, and that the female-bearing eggs cannot carry the grossidariata factor. Thus in mating no. 4 above (using G and L for gross, and lact), the heterozygous {G (L)) female produces male eggs bearing G and female eggs bearing L ; all the spermatozoa (germ-cells) of the male bear L, and thus the offspring are GL (= gross.) males and LL (= lact.) females. But in the converse cross no. 3, the lact. female (LL) produces male and female eggs both bearing L, the heterozygous (GL) male produces equal numbers of G and L spermatozoa, and thus the offspring are GL and LL males, GL and LL females. We thus arrive at the conclusion that

124 HEREDITY [CH.

in this species at least the sex is determined by the egg of the female parent, even before fertilisation.

Perhaps the most remarkable fact in the whole case remains to be mentioned, that, as far as experi- ment has shown, pure (homozygous) females of the moth A. grossulariata do not exist; all the wild and apparently pure gross, females that have been tested by pairing with lact. males have given lact. female and gross, male offspring, so showing that they are in constitution GL. Since, however, wild males are pure gross. (GG) and since GL females produce (j-bearing male eggs and iy-bearing female eggs, the lact. (L) factor never normally becomes transferred to the male sex. Similar cases have been discovered in the inheritance of pink eyes in Canaries, and more recently for more than one pair of characters in Fowls. Of the latter, the most noteworthy is the barring of the Plymouth Rock, for which all barred hens are found to be heterozygous when mated with an unbarred (black) cock.

The essential feature of the explanation given above is that eggs of two kinds, male-producing and female-producing, are laid in equal numbers, and that the character for femaleness cannot be associated in the same egg with the gross, factor, so that female- bearing eggs must always bear lact. The case is probably comparable with the 'gametic coupling' which has been found between characters belonging

IX] HEREDITY AND SEX 125

to diiferent allelomorphic pairs in the sweet-pea and other flowers, quite apart from sex. Sweet-pea flowers are of two shapes, with 'erect' standards or with 'hooded' standards. The erect character is dominant to the hooded. Among the off^spring of certain sweet-peas heterozygous for this pair of characters and also for purple and red flowers, the purple ofispring include both erect and hooded standards, but the reds are nearly all erect. This means that in the formation of the germ-cells the purple factor and the erect factor go into diflferent germ-cells, so that instead of four kinds of germ- cells being produced in equal numbers, two are produced in great excess, viz. purple-hooded and red- erect. These meeting at random give purple-hooded, purple-erect and red-erect, but hardly any red-hooded.

It has recently been discovered by Bateson and Punnett that the purple and erect factors go chiefly into difierent germ-cells if introduced into the heterozygote by diflerent parents, but are ' coupled ' together and go into the same germ-cell if introduced from the same parent. In the case of the Currant Moth the G character is always introduced into the female from the male and possibly for this reason is never borne by female-bearing eggs.

It has been seen that the case of the Currant Moth and the similar cases in Fowls and Canaries lead to the inevitable conclusion that in these animals

126 HEREDITY [ch.

there are two kinds of eggs, male-bearing and female- bearing. In other cases, however, exactly similar evidence shows that the germ-cells of the male (spermatozoa) are of two kinds. The example of this which has been most fully worked out is the Fly Drosophila, in which a series of remarkable dis- coveries have been made in America by Morgan. He has found several sex-limited characters, perhaps the simplest of which is eye-colour. The normal flies have red eyes ; a white-eyed male appeared and when mated with a red-eyed female gave only red-eyed oflfepring, both males and females. When, however, these were mated together, all the female oflPspring were red-eyed, but about half the males had white eyes. A white-eyed male mated with a heterozygous red- eyed female gave red and white eyes in both sexes, but any red-eyed male mated with a white- eyed female gave all the males white, all the females red- eyed. This case then is in every respect precisely the converse of what is found in the Currant Moth ; the males must be heterozygous for a sex-determiner, and the red factor in the male is transmitted only to the female offspring.

Apparently similar conditions are found in Man in certain diseases of the eye, e.g. colour-blindness and certain forms of night-blindness, and in the disease known as Haemophilia. Here the affected male appears to transmit the factor for the disease only to

IX] HEREDITY AND SEX 127

his daughters, for sons of aflected men are normal and do not transmit, while the daughters of affected men, though not themselves affected, transmit the disease to some of their sons.

The suggested explanation is that ' normality ' is represented by a factor for which the female is homozygous (NN), the male heterozygous (Nil). If in the male the normality factor N is replaced by the factor for colour-blindness, for example (C), it will cause the appearance of the disease in the male for there is no second N to counteract C. The male transmits N or C only to his daughters, but a woman who receives C from her father will receive N from her mother, and so will not be colour-blind. She will transmit C, however, to half her children, so that some of her sons will be colour-blind. This is illus- trated in the following scheme.

? NN X ^ On

normal woman //\ colour-blind man

X \

%CN X ^ Nil

heterozygous /\ normal man woman y \

%CN'%NN ^ Cn J^i

In the fly Drosophila and in Man, then, it is clear that the germ-cells of the male must be of two kinds, while we have seen that precisely similar evidence

128 HEREDITY [ch.

from a moth and two kinds of birds shows that in them the female produces two kinds of germ-cells.

The conclusion has been drawn by some students of the subject that in the Moth, Canary and Fowl the egg determines the sex of the offspring, while in Man and the fly Drosophila the spermatozoon determines the sex. There is some evidence, however, that both modes of inheritance may exist in Fowls, which would contradict this supposition, and for this and other reasons it is perhaps more likely that in animals generally, and probably in plants also, there are two kinds of egg-cells (germ-cells produced by the female) and two kinds of spermatozoa (germ-cells of the male) and that an egg-cell destined to produce a female is fertilised by a spermatozoon of one kind, a male- producing egg by a spermatozoon of the other kind. The facts in any case indicate that the determiners for maleness and femaleness are comparable with Mendelian characters, which segregate from each other in the formation of the germ-cells as do the 'factors' for Mendelian allelomorphs. For a more detailed presentation of this hypothesis and the evidence on which it rests, the reader must be referred to the works cited in the bibliography [especially 2, 9a, 226, 43a].

It will be noticed that in the description of the nature of sex-determination here barely outlined, the sex of an individual is regarded as being no less

ixl HEREDITY AND SEX 129

irrevocably determined by the factors contained in the germ-cells than is the appearance of an inherited Mendelian character. This view is opposed to the older belief that the sex is determined by nutrition and other environmental conditions acting on the embryo or larva. It may be well therefore to summarise the evidence for the belief that sex is essentially inherited, and not determined by conditions. The evidence is of several distinct kinds. In the first place, in a variety of animals (and in Man in the case of 'identical twins'), whenever one egg divides to form two or more individuals, these are always of tiie same sex. Secondly, several animals belonging to widely diverse groups produce two kinds of eggs, one yielding males, the other females, and in these cases the sex is clearly determined before development begins. Thirdly, in the Bee and other forms, a fertilised egg yields females, an unfertilised egg, males ; in this case fertilisation appears to determine the sex, but condi- tions operating later have no power to change it. In all these widely distributed cases we have direct evidence that sex is determined from the fertilisation of the egg or before it, presumably by 'sex-deter- minants' present in the germ-cells.

Evidence of a different nature has been afforded by the study of the development of the germ-cells, especially but not exclusively in Insects. In the nucleus of the developing cell in a number of species

D. 9

130 HEREDITY [ch.

it is found that the bodies known as chromosomes (see Appendix, pp. 140, 146) have an even number in the female and one less in the male, for example in one case 22 in the female and 21 in the male. In the female two of these difler visibly from the rest, in the male there is one odd one, the remaining twenty being like the corresponding twenty of the female. At the * maturation divisions,' when the chromosome number is halved, 11 go into each mature egg, but in the male the odd one goes into half the spermatozoa, leav- ing the other half without it. All eggs thus contain 10 + 1, but spermatozoa are formed in equal numbers having 10+1 and 10. In fertilisation then two kinds of zygotes will be produced, those made by (10 + 1) egg + (10 + 1) spermatozoon giving 20 + 2 (female), and those made by (10 + 1) -,"- 10 giving 20 + 1 (male). On the hypothesis of the nature of sex outlined above it is supposed that the odd chromosomes are related to the sex-determinants (if they are not themselves the determinants), so that the sex is determined by the combination of chromosomes in the nucleus. In any case, since the numbers are regularly found, external conditions can have no part in deciding the sex of the individual.

Finally, the cases of relation between sex and in- herited characters described above (lactieolor, colour- blindness etc.) leave no room for the action of environment after the individual has once begun to

IX] HEREDITY AND SEX 131

develop. But it should be pointed out that environ- ment may influence the proportion of male-producing and female-producing germ-cells which come to maturity, and a considerable amount of evidence has been collected showing that the varying proportions of the sexes under different conditions may be due to this cause. If some conditions favour the output of male-producing eggs, others of female-producing, the conditions will then indirectly affect the sex of the offspring, not by changing it in the individual, but by causing more individuals of one or the other sex to be born.

One of the reasons which have led biologists to assume that environment may modify the sex of the individual is the fact that changed conditions may influence the extent to which the sexual characters develop. Worker bees for example are females in which modified food and surroundings have prevented the full development of the female characters. The characters proper to one sex frequently are found in a rudimentary condition in the other, and in abnormal cases may develop. Or these characters may be prevented from developing in the sex which nor- mally exhibits them by certain conditions, especially by removing the ovary or testis from the young animal, for example the non-appearance of horns in castrated deer or the disappearance of the distinctive sex-features in Crabs in which the reproductive organs

9—2

132 HEREDITY [cH.

are destroyed by parasites \ Cases of this kind there- fore bring us back to the question of 'secondary sexual characters ' and sexual dimorphism which were briefly touched upon at the beginning of this chapter.

In discussing the determination of sex, the nature of sex itself must be sharply distinguished from the secondary characters which are normally associated with it. The essential difference between the sexes is the presence of testes in the male and ovaries in the female ; the associated characters, which may or may not be directly connected with reproduction, are certainly sometimes and may perhaps always be clearly separated into a distinct category. It has been seen that the evidence is conclusive that sex often, and probably always, is determined by the germ-cells, i.e. from the moment of fertilisation if not before. But it is equally clear that the characters associated with one or the other sex are not de- termined in the same way ; their presence or the extent to which they are developed may be to some extent dependent upon environment. Tliis is shown by the case of the worker-bee ; by the effects of cas- tration in preventing the appearance of distinctively sexual characters ; by the appearance in old or

1 In this particular instance it has been found that a male crab affected by the parasite may even produce eggs, and it must be supposed that femaleness is present in a recessive condition, so to speak, in the male, and is caused to appear by the action of the parasite (Sacculina).

IX] HEREDITY AND SEX 133

sterile female birds of plumage resembling that of the male ; and lastly by the occasional development in one sex of characters which are normally rudi- mentary in that sex, but are well developed in the other, such as the breasts in man. If such characters are not directly connected with reproduction, for example the splendid plumage of some male birds or the horns of deer, they are called secondary sexual characters, but there is no sharp distinction between these and accessory sexual structures such as the egg- laying apparatus of female insects.

It was said above that the appearance of sexual characters is to some extent dependent on environ- ment, but in fact the only changes which can modify their development are those Avhich aifect the essential reproductive organs, the ovaries and testes. When these are removed or suppressed, the sexual characters may be modified ; their appearance indeed depends in many cases upon the presence of functional sexual organs, and frequently they do not appear until the sexual organs are mature. Now it has been seen above that certain characters are dominant in one sex and recessive in the other, and the most typical case of this is in an actual secondary sexual character horns of sheep, for when horns are present they are always better developed in the male. Their 'recessive' condition in a ewe produced by crossing a horned by a hornless breed must probably be ascribed to a

134 HEREDITY [CH.

factor, confined to the female, which prevents horns from developing even though the factor for horned- ness is present. Some such inhibiting factor, confined to one sex, may possibly explain sexual dimorphism in general. It may be of course that in some cases the inheritance of sexual characters is limited like that of the variety lacticolor in the Currant Moth ; if the var. lacticolor were dominant instead of recessive, we should have an instance of ordinary sexual dimorphism in this case, for all females are normally heterozygous and would therefore show one character, all males homozygous, and would show the other. Some con- dition of this kind will probably be found to apply to the remarkable cases of certain butterflies which have two or three discontinuous and very distinct forms of the female, but only one kind of male, and in which all the different forms have been bred from eggs laid by one female insect. The few breeding experiments that have yet been made with these species (Papilios or * Swallow-tails ' from Africa and India) suggest that a combination of alternative dominance and sex- limited inheritance will probably be found to explain them. It seems reasonable to suppose, therefore, that not only the determination of sex itself, but the difficult problems of sexual dimorphism and the inheritance of secondary sexual characters, will all be found on analysis to fall into line with the simple principles of Mendelian Heredity.

135

APPENDIX I

HISTORICAL SUMMARY OF THEORIES OF HEREDITY

In the foregoing pages, Heredity has been regarded as the relation between parents and offspring in respect of their bodily characters, and it has been shown that this relation has been brought about in some way by the germ-cells, but very little has been said about the mechanism by which this is effected. This side of the question is very largely speculative, and in order to keep speculation to some extent apart from facts, an account of theories of hereditary transmission, and of recent work on the supposed material basis of hereditary characters, has been postponed to appendices, which the reader who seeks for facts and well-founded deductions alone, may omit at will. First, a summary of the chief theories of heredity will be given, and then a short account of recent work on the subject.

In the introduction it was pointed out how closely related are our ideas of Heredity and Variation with

136 HEREDITY [ap.

theories of Evolution, and thus the history of the two subjects largely coincides. The first important theory was that of Lamarck, published in 1809, and although it had little influence at the time, in more recent years Lamarck's main principle has found many sup- porters. His theory was essentially that ' acquired ' modifications are being continually produced and perfected by every organism during its life, and that they are at least partially transmitted to its ofispring, so that each generation will be rather better adapted to its surroundings than its predecessor. In this way, for example, the great length of the neck of the girafie would be explained by the continual striving through many generations to reach higher leaves on the trees ; or the limbless condition of snakes and slow-worms by the gradual loss of limbs through disuse. But it has been seen that the assumption that acquired characters are inherited is open to grave doubt, and hence the followers of Lamarck are fewer at the present time than formerly.

Darwin's great theory of Evolution by Natural Selection of course depends on quite different prin- ciples, but it, like Lamarck's, is based essentially upon the laws of variation and heredity. Darwin himself made astonishing progress in the investigation of these laws, and although he would doubtless have been the first to admit the incompleteness of our knowledge, yet he collecLed sufficient evidence to

ij THEORIES OF HEREDITY 137

enable him to formulate one of the first really impor- tant theories of heredity, which he called the Theory of Pangenesis [7, (1868)]. The essence of his theory was that every cell of an organism gives off minute particles or ' Gemmules ' from itself, which circulate in the body and finally come to rest in the germ-cells, or in parts where buds may be developed. The gemmules were regarded as being capable of multi- plication, and of transmission to a future generation in a dormant state. They were supposed to be given off from all tissues at every stage of development, so that every unit of the organism at every stage would be represented in the germ-cells. On the develop- ment of the germ-cell, the contained gemmules would give rise to cells like those from which they were derived, and so the characters of one generation would be transmitted to those which follow.

By this hypothesis Darwin accounted for the phenomena of sexual and non-sexual reproduction, regeneration of lost parts, variability, inheritance both of inborn and acquired characters, and lastly of reversion to a previous ancestor. The hypothesis was one of the first which attempted to bring all these various groups of facts into line, but it had the serious defect that there was no direct evidence whatever for the existence of gemmules, and, assuming their existence, to be accommodated in the germ-cells they must be so exceedingly minute as to be almost

138 HEREDITY [ap.

unimaginable. The Theory of Pangenesis never gained any very wide acceptance, but is of great importance owing to its stimulating effect on later work and thought. To a gi-eat extent it led to the formulation of other theories of heredity \ any account of which is prevented by limitation of space. It can only be mentioned that the chief hypotheses which followed Darwin's laid successively more and more emphasis upon the idea that the germ-cells are not made up of samples taken from the body, but have a certain independence. So gi-ew up the conception of 'germinal continuity,' that is, the idea that the germ-cell of one generation gives rise not only to the body of the next, but also directly to its germ-ceils, so that the body does not produce germ-cells, but only contains them. We must now turn to the theory in which this idea finds its most celebrated expression, AVeismann's Theory of the Germ-2)lasm (1885) [40, 8].

It is impossible in a short space to give an adequate account of Weismann's great theory, which he has worked out in fuller minuteness of detail than has been done with any other theory of heredity, and by which he has done more to stimulate discussion and research than perhaps any biologist since Darwin.

^ For a summary of the more important theories of heredity, especially those of Herbert Spencer (1863, i.e. before Darwin's theory of Pangenesis), Galton (1875) and de Viied (1889), see Thomson's Heredity [33]. See also [8].

I] THEORIES OF HEREDITY 139

Weismann was led by his work on the origin of the germ-cells to a belief in germinal continuity as explained above, but the facts of regeneration of lost parts and other related phenomena caused him to give up the idea that a sharp distinction could be drawn between the cells of the body and the germ, and to substitute for it the idea of a distinction between body-substance and germ-substance, or as he calls it, body-plasm and germ-plasm. According to this hypothesis, the egg contains germ-plasm derived from that of the parent, and as the egg develops the germ-plasm increases and becomes distributed among the cells, and gradually, as the cells become specialised to form the different parts, the germ-plasm becomes converted into body-plasm and builds up the varied kinds of ceUs of the body. But some cells continue to possess the full complement of ancestral germ-plasm, and these will go to form the germ-cells of the next generation. When an organ remains capable of regenerating lost parts, it is assumed that germ-plasm having the power to develop such parts remains in the cells and becomes active when required- Germ-plasm can thus be converted into body-plasm, but body-plasm cannot become germ-plasm, and hence Weismann assumes that no change brought about in the body (by environment, etc.) but not affecting the germ-cells, can be inherited by subsequent generations. It is

140 HEREDITY [ap.

therefore impossible according to his theory, that 'acquired characters' in the technical sense should ever be inherited. The germ-plasm of one genera- tion gives origin to the germ-plasm of the next, and no external conditions acting on the body which contains and nourishes the germ-plasm can have effects which are transmitted unless the germ-plasm itself is altered.

Weismann in a series of books and papers has built up a detailed and highly complicated and speculative scheme of the nature and composition of the germ-plasm, only a brief summary of which can be given here. Much of it will doubtless not stand the test of fuller investigation, and parts of it are already discredited ; but it has had the merit of stimulating an immense amount of valuable re- search, and there are indications that some of his fundamental ideas will form the foundations of a true theory of the material basis of heredity.

Weismann assumes that the germ-plasm is con- tained in the nucleus of the cell, and, in particular, in the bodies known as chromosomes. Every nucleus contains a number of these bodies, in the ordinary condition of the nucleus distributed through its substance so as to be unrecognisable, but when the cell is about to divide they make their appearance as rod-like bodies whose number in general is con- stant in the nuclei of the same species of animal or

I] THEORIES OF HEREDITY 141

plant. Before the nucleus divides the chromosomes split longitudinally so that they are accurately halved and the two halves of each go into different daughter- nuclei. Weismann supposes that the germ-plasm is contained in the chromosomes, and consists of numerous units with different properties. When the chromosome splits, each unit is supposed to divide into two similar halves, and thus each daughter- nucleus receives a similar complement of germ-plasm. In the union of male and female cells in fertilisa- tion, the nucleus of each cell brings its complement of chromosomes, and thus if there were no special provision, the number of chromosomes would be doubled in each generation. But it is actually found that in the cell-divisions immediately preceding the development of both male and female sex-cells, a process occurs which results in the removal of half the chromosomes from the nucleus, and thus when the male and female nuclei unite the normal number of chromosomes for the species is restored. Since Weismann regards the chromosomes as consisting of germ-plasm, and as made up of a vast number of units, each of which is the determinant for one hereditary character, he saw that, without some such process of removal of chromosomes in the formation of the sex-cells, the germ-plasm must in a few genera- tions become infinitely complicated. He therefore predicted that some process of 'reduction' of chromo-

142 HEREDITY [at.

somes must occur, either by elimination of complete chromosomes or by transverse instead of longitudinal splitting, before any complete observations had been made showing that this actually happens.

Since Weismann supposes that the germ-plasm is contained in the chromosomes of the germ-cells, and since half the chromosomes are removed in the 'maturation' of these cells without preventing the transmission of any part to the offspring by inherit- ance, he concluded that each chromosome contains aU the units (' determinants ') necessary to a complete individual. (Later work has rendered this conclusion doubtfid : see Appendix ii.) When fusion of male and female sex-cells takes place, the resulting individual will contain a mixture of the parental germ-plasms, the paternal in some chromosomes, the maternal in others. In the maturation of the sex-cells half these germ-plasms will be removed and in the next generation a fresh mixture will take place. It thus follows that the different chromosomes contain germ- plasms descended from different ancestors, and different mixtures of these will occur in different individuals. Here then we come to Weismann's hypothesis of the origin of variation. Since different individuals contain different combinations of an- cestral germ-plasms, these wiU lead to varying effects in the development of the body ; new com- binations will be continually occurring in every

I] THEORIES OF HEREDITY 143

fertilised egg, and thus arises the variation between separate individuals. Further, although by his theory changes brought about in the body-plasm cannot be transferred to the germ-plasm, yet influences acting on the germ-plasm itself may modify it and so their effects will be transmitted. The most important of these influences is nourishment, which may favour some units of the germ-plasm rather than others. He further supposes that there may be competition for nourishment among the difierent units ('deter- minants') so that some increase at the expense of others, and if this process should be continued through a series of generations, certain characters would show a steady increase while others corre- spondingly decrease. Variation thus arises by changes brought about in the germ-plasm, and by the recombination of varied ancestral germ-plasms in each generation. Such variations wiU be inherited, and in this respect will differ entirely from changes brought about in the body during its life by the action of environment.

It has been shown that in the earlier theories of heredity it was assumed that the germ-cells were produced by the body, and that they must therefore be supposed either to contain samples of all parts of it, or at least some kind of units derived from those parts and able to cause their development in the next generation. Gradually, as the study

144 HEREDITY [ap. i

of heredity and of the actual origin of the germ-cells has progressed, biologists have given up this view in favour of a belief in germinal continuity, that is, that the germ-substance is derived from previous germ-substance, the body being a kind of offshoot fi'om it. The child is thus like its parent, not because it is produced from the parent, but because both child and parent are produced irom. the same stock of germ-plasm.

145

APPENDIX II

THE MATERIAL BASIS OF INHERITANCE

In Appendix i, it has been mentioned that Weismann regarded the chromosomes of the nucleus as the bearers of hereditary characters, and more recent work has added to the probability of this view, while not as yet providing anything which can be called proof. That some, if not all, the hereditary characters are determined by the nucleus of the germ-cell is indicated by several facts. In the first place, the spermatozoon consists of little else but a nucleus with a vibrating tail, and the tail may be shed as the spermatozoon enters the ovum. Secondly, experiments in fertilising non-nucleated fragments of sea-urchin eggs by sperm of a different species, give evidence that the hereditary characters of the resulting larvae are exclusively those of the paternal species. This conclusion however has been disputed, and can only be regarded as probable rather than certain. Again, experiments in fertilising one egg simultaneously by more than one spermatozoon, lead

D. 10

146 HEREDITY [ap.

Boveri [3] to believe that the subsequent develop- ment of the cells of the embryo depends on the distribution of the chromosomes in the abnormal divisions consequent on double fertilisation. And Herbst [16] has obtained sea-urchin larvae made by crossing distinct species, which on one side of the body resemble one parent, and on the other side the other parent. He shows that these differences are connected with differences in the size of the nuclei of the two sides, and that probably the part with maternal characters contains only maternal nuclear substance, while the part showing the paternal character has nuclei derived from both parents.

But probably the best evidence for regarding the chromosomes as bearing the essential determinants for hereditary characters is provided by the behaviour of the chromosomes themselves in the maturation divisions of the germ-cells. It has been pointed out that at these nuclear divisions the chromosome number is halved, and restored to the fuU number again in the next generation by the union of two germ-cells each bearing the half-number. Now it has been found in certain cases that the chromosomes are not all alike, but differ among themselves in size and shape, and when this is so it can be seen that the nucleus just before maturation contains two of each kind. If the different kinds of chromosomes

II] MATERIAL BASIS 147

are represented by letters, A, B, C, D..., there will then be two A'b, two ^'s, etc. in the nucleus. The actual processes in the reduction division are some- what complex, but briefly they consist in a pairing together of the chromosomes, followed by a division of the nucleus in which the two members of each pair are separated into diflerent daughter-nuclei, so that the daughter-nuclei each contain half the full number. When the chromosomes differ among themselves, it is seen that two similar ones always pair together, i.e. A with A, B with B, etc. Thus the daughter-nuclei each contain the whole series A, B, C. . ., but have only one of each, instead of two. If then it is imagined that each chromosome is the bearer of the determinant (or 'factor') for a Mendelian character, we may regard one individual as having a double series of chromosomes A, B, C. . ., etc., and another as bearing the allelomorphic cha^ racters a, b, c..., etc. When these individuals are mated, the heterozygote will bear both series, A and a, B and b, etc. In the formation of the germ-cells, A will segregate from a, B from b in exactly the way required by Mendelian theory. But there is no reason to suppose the series A, B, C... should all go into one germ-cell, and a, b,c... into the other ; A may go into the first daughter-nucleus and a into the second, but b may go with A into the first, and B into the second. So in crossing races differing

10—2

148 HEREDITY [ap.

in more than one allelomorphic pair, all possible combinations can be produced, except that no germ- cell can contain both the members of one pair.

The suggestion that this segregation of chromo- somes, which can be seen to take place, is the mechanism by which the members of an allelo- morphic pair of characters are segregated, is quite speculative; but it seems exceedingly unlikely that machinery so exactly adapted to bring it about should be found in every developing germ-cell, if it had no connexion with the segregation of characters that is observed in experimental breeding. There is also the further fact in support of the suggestion, that it is known in many insects that one pair of chromo- somes is closely connected with sex, for in the males of these species one chromosome of the pair is absent or much reduced, but in the female both are similar. These sex-chromosomes separate from one another like the others (when both are present), and it has been seen that there is experimental evidence for the view that the sex-determiners behave like Mendelian allelomorphs. One serious diflSiculty however suggests itself at once ; the chromosomes are limited in number, and it is undoubted that more allelomorphic pairs of characters may exist in a species than there are pairs of chromosomes, although in such cases there is no evidence that members of different pairs are always associated together. Several suggestions have been

II] MATERIAL BASIS 149

made to meet this difficulty, of which perhaps the most adequate is that the chromosomes are not in- divisible entities, but are composed of smaller units, each of which corresponds with one Mendelian factor. The chromosomes are not permanently present in the distinct form which is seen during cell-division, but during the resting condition of the nucleus their substance becomes diffused over a network of threads, only to be collected again into definite chromosomes, having the same number and form as before, pre- paratory to the next division. If each chromosome consists of a series of units having a definite arrange- ment, and these units become scattered in the 'resting phase,' but are re-collected in the same order when the chromosomes are re-formed, it does not seem unlikely that a unit N may take the place of the corresponding unit n from the other chromo- some of the pair, so that if the chromosome A consisted at one division of units M, N, O..., and the corresponding chromosome a consisted of m, n, o..., after the resting stage N and n might have exchanged places, and chromosome A would consist of M, n, O. . and a of m, N, o.... By some process of this kind it seems probable that the observed phenomena of chromosome reduction would account for all the facts of Mendelian segregation.

It must be stated quite clearly, however, that the study of the possible relation between chromo-

150 HEREDITY [ap. ii

somes and body-characters is as yet in its infancy ; and this brief note can only sketch the lines on which modern work seems to support Weismann's hypo- thesis that the chromosomes are the physical basis of inheritance. It will be seen that his suggestion that all the chromosomes are on the whole similar is not confirmed, but the evidence that chromosomes do bear factors for inherited characters is consider- ably stronger than when the idea was first put forward \

^ The suggestion referred to in the note on p. 97 must also be borne in mind, that it is not the factor ' alone which determines the development of a character, but a physiological relation between the factor contained in a chromosome and the surrounding protoplasm. If the latter is altered from any cause, the relation may be changed and the character modified, just as the plants raised from one sample of seed may be modified by growing them in different soils. If, as is quite possible, the relation is a reciprocal one, the factor may in some cases be permanently modified, and there would thus be a mechanism for the transmission of acquired modifications. At present, however, it must be admitted that experiment hardly provides sufiQcient basis for speculations such as these.

151

LITERATURE LIST

The Works marked * are general treatises suitable for further study.

1. Bateson, W. Materials for the Study of Variation. London, 1894.

*2. Mendel's Principles of Heredity. 2nd Impression.

Cambridge, 1909. (Full bibliographyto date of publication.)

3. Boveri, T. Ergebnisse iiber die Konstitution der Chroma-

tischen Substanz des Zellkerns. Jena, 1904.

4. Castle, W. E. Heredity of Hair-length in Guinea-pigs, and

its Bearing on the Theory of Pure Gametes. Publ. Carnegie Inst. Washington, No. 49, 1906.

5. Studies of Inheritance in Rabbits. Publ. Carnegie

Inst No. 114, 1909.

*5a. Darbishire, A. D. Breeding and the Mendelian Discovery.

Cassell, 1911. *6. Darwin, C. The Origin of Species.

*7. Variation of Animals and Plants under Domestication.

*8. Darwin and Modern Science. Cambridge, 1909. 9. Davenport, C.B. Statistical Methods with Special Reference

to Biological Variation. New York and London, 1899. 9a. Doncaster, L. Recent work on the Determination of Sex.

Science Progress, July 1909: reprinted in Smithsonian

Report (Washington) for 1910.

152 LITERATURE LIST

10. Eugenics Laboratory. Publications of the Galton Laboratory

for National Eugenics, University of London :

Especially, Elderton, E. M. Relative strength of Nature and Nurture. Heron, D. A First Study of the Statistics of Insanity. Schuster, E. Inheritance of Ability. Bar- ring-ton, A. and Pearson, K. A Preliminary Study of Extreme Alcoholism in Adults.

11. E wart, J. C, The Penycuik Experiments. London, 1899. *12. Galton, P. Inquiries into Human Faculty and its Develop- ment. Macmillan, 1883. (Cheap Edition, J. M. Dent.)

*13. Natural Inheritance. Macmillan, 1889.

14. Essays in Eugenics. Eugenics Education Soc. Lon- don, 1909. *15. Hereditary Genius. Macmillan, 1869.

16. Herbst, C. Vererbungsstudien, i vi. Archiv fiir Entwick-

lungsmechanik, 1906 1909. Especially v vi, 1907 and 1909.

17. Hurst, C. C. On the Inheritance of Eye-colour in Man.

Proc. Roy. Soc. (B) Vol. 80. 1908.

18. Johannsen, W. Ueber Erblichkeit in Populationen und

in Reinen Linien. Jena, 1903. 18a. Journal of Genetics, 1911 A quarterly journal containing irapoi-tant papei's on Heredity and allied subjects, edited by Professors Bateson and Punnett. Camb. Univ. Press.

19. Kammerer, P. Vererbung erzwungener Fortpflanzungsan-

passungen. Arch, f Entwicklungsmechanik, Vols, xxv

(1907) and xxviii (1909). *20. Kellogg, V. L. Darwinism Today. London and New York,

1907. *21. Lock, R. H. Recent Progress in the Study of Variation,

Heredity and Evolution. 2nd Edition. Murray, 1909. *22. Lotzy, J. P. Vorlesungeu iiber Descendenztheorien. Jena,

1906.

LTTimATlIRE LIST 153

22rt. M''Dougall, D. T. Organic Response. American Naturalist,

Vol. 45, p. 5, Jan. 1911. 22b. Morgan, T. 11. Nvnnerous papers in 'Science,' 'American

Natiu'alist' and 'Journal of Experimental Zoology,' iniO-

II. 23. Nettleship, E. Bowman Lecture. Trans. Ophthalm. Soc.

1909. *24. Pearson, K. The Grammar of Science. 3rd Edition, Part ii.

London, 1912.

25. Mathematical Contributions to the Theory of Evolution.

Proc. and Trans. Roy. Soc. (A) 1896 1903; also numerous papers in Biometrika, 1902 , especially 'On the Laws of Inheritance in Man,' 1903, 1904; and 'The Law of Ancestral Heredity,' 1903.

26. Huxley Lecture of Anthropologicallnst. of Gt. Britain

and Ireland. Trans. Anthrop. Inst. 1903, p. 179.

*26«. Przbram, H. Exi^erimental Zoologie. Vol. in. Phylogenese. *27. Punnett, R. C. Mendelism. 3rd Edition. Macmillan, 1911. *28. Reid, Archdall. The Principles of Heredity. London, 1906. *29. Romanes, J. G. Darwin and After Darwin. 3 vols. London, 1892—1897.

30. Royal Society. Reports to the Evolution Committee, i v,

1902—1909.

31. Schuster, E. Hereditary Deafness. Biometrika, iv, 1906,

p. 465.

32. Standfuss. Handbuch der Palaarktischen Grossschmetter-

linge. Jena, 1896. 32a. Sumner, F. B. An Experimental Study of Somatic

Modifications. Arch. f. Entwicklungsmechanik, Vol. xxx,

Pt. II, p. 317, and American Naturalist, XLVi, p. 5. *33. Thomson, J. A. Heredity. Murray, 1908. (Very good

bibliography.) 34. Tower. An Investigation of Evolution in Chrysomelid

154 LITERATURE LIST

Beetles of the Genus Leptinotarsa. Publ. Caniegie Inst. Washington, No. 48, 1906. 35. Treasury of Human Inheritance. Publ. Gralton Laboratory for Nat. Eugenics, London University. *36. Vries, H. de. The Mutation Theory (Trans.). London, 1910.

*37. Species and Varieties, their origin through Mutation.

Chicago and Loudon, 1905. *38. Wallace, A. K Darwinism. London, 1889. 39. Weisniann, A. Essays upon Heredity and Kindred Subjects (Trans.). Oxford, 1891, 1892.

*40. The Germ Plasm (Trans. Parker and Ronnfeldt).

London, 1893.

*41. The Evolution Theory (Trans. J. A. and M. R.

Thomson). London, 1904. 42. Wheldale, Muriel. Plant Oxydases and the Chemical Inter- relationships of Colour-varieties. Progressus Rei Botani- cae, ni, p. 457. Jena, 1910. *43. Whetham, W. C. D. and C. The Family and the Nation. London, 1910. 43rt. Wilson, E. B. Studies on Chromosomes. Journal of Exp.

Zoology. 1905-1909. 44. Yule, G. U. Mendel's Laws and their probable relations to intra-racial Heredity. New Phytologist. Vol. i, 1902, p. 193.

155

GLOSSARY

Acquired Character. A feature developed during the life of the individual possessing it, in response to the action of use or environment.

Albino. An animal without pigment in the skin, hair or eyes. The hair is white ; the eyes pink ovnng to the colour of the blood. Among plants, white-flowered varieties are called albinos. The condition is called albinism.

Allelomorph. One of a pair of alternative Mendelian characters. When a pair of characters are alternative in their in- heritance, and segregate from each other in the formation of the germ-cells of an individual which contains both, they are said to be allelomorphic with each other. See Segregation.

Anther. The part of the stamen in a flower which contains the pollen.

Chromogen. A colourless substance which when oxidised gives rise to a coloured body (pigment).

Chromosome. A body in the nucleus of a cell, which absorbs stains and becomes clearly visible during nuclear division, but be- comes dispersed through the nucleus during the resting phase. During nuclear division each chromosome becomes accurately halved, so that in general all cells of each species of animal or plant contain an equal number of chromosomes.

Continuovis. See Variation.

Determinant. The hypothetical unit in a germ-cell which determines the production of a i^articular character in the individual derived from that germ-cell. See Factor.

156 GLOSSARY

Deviation. The amount by which an individual differs from the mode in continuous variation.

Dimorphism. The condition in which a species exists in two distinct types or sharply separable varieties. When the two sexes differ thus, the condition is called Sexual Dimorphism.

Dominant. When two varieties, differing in one character, are crossed together, and all the offspring have the character borne by one parent, that character is dominant Applied to one of a pair of Mendelian allelomorphs.

Egg-cell. The germ-cell produced by the female.

Epistatic. When one character A is superposed upon another B, so that A prevents or obscures the appearance of B, al- though they are not allelomorphic with each other, A is said to be epistatic to B.

^Extracted' Hom,ozygote. When two heterozygous individuals are mated together, their homozygous offspring are spoken of as ' extracted ' homozygotes.

i^i, F^. The Symbol Fi is used to indicate the offspring (first filial generation) of a mating between two differing individuals. The later generations (second, third filial, etc.) are repre- sented by Fi, F3, etc.

Factor. In Mendelian inheritance, the hereditary determinant (q.v.) of a particular character is spoken of as the factor for that character.

Ferment. A body which has the power of causing chemical action between substances which in its absence are inactive towards one another.

Fertilisation. The union of male and female germ-cells which precedes the development of a new individual. It consists essentially in the fusion of the nuclei of the gei-m-cells.

Gamete. A germ-cell, q.v.

Gemmules. Hypothetical bodies supposed to be given off by the cells of the body, and entering the gei-m-cells, to transmit heritable characters to the next generation.

GLOSSARY 157

Germ-cell. A reproductive cell, which, usually after union with a germ-cell from another individual (fertilisation), develops into a new individual. In animals the germ-cells of the male are spermatozoa, those of the female ova {egg-cells). In plants the male germ-cells are contained in the pollen ; the female, egg-cells, in the ovules or embryo-seeds.

Germ-plasm. The germinal substance, which according to Weismann is alone able to give origin to new individuals.

Heterozygote. An individual containing both members of an allelomorphic pair of characters, i.e. which is hybrid in respect of that pair of characters, and produces germ-cells bearing one and the other respectively. Adjective heterozygous.

Homozygote. An individual made by union of two germ-cells each of which bears the same member of an allelomorpliic pair of characters, so that it is 'pure' in respect of that character, and all its germ-cells bear the same character. Adjective homozygous.

Mode. The most frequent condition of a character which varies continuously. Its measurement is called the modal value.

Mutation. A variety which is not connected with the type by intermediates. More strictly, the sudden origin of such a variety.

Nucleus. A sharply defined body found in every cell, which seems to control the activities of the cell.

Ovum. The germ-cell produced by the female, an egg-cell.

Pin-eye. In Primula (Primrose, Cowslip, etc.), the form in which the style is long and the anthers low down in the flower-tube. The other form, with short style and anthers high up, is called Thrum-eye.

Pollen. The powder bearing the male germ-cells in a flowering plant.

Polym,orphism. The condition in which a species exists in several distinct forms or varieties.

Recessive. When two individuals are crossed, bearing diflcrent

158 GLOSSARY

members of an allelomorphic pair of characters, the member of the pair which does not appear in the offspring is called recessive.

Reversion. A ' throw-back ' to a previous ancestor, or to the type of the species, when varieties are crossed.

Segregation. In Mendelian inheritance, the separation of the two characters of an allelomorphic pair, in a heterozygote, into distinct germ-cells, i.e. the formation of gametes each bearing one character of a pair, by an individual which contains both members.

Self -fertilisation. The fertilisation of a female gamete by a male gamete produced from the same individual The process in plants is spoken of shortly as selfing.

Somatic. Having reference to the body {'soma') considered as distinct from the germ-cells. A character borne or exhibited by the body but not represented in the germ-cells is called somatic as contrasted with germ.inal.

Spermatozoon. See Germ-Cell.

Style. The part of a flower which receives the pollen, and con- ducts the male germ-cell to the egg-cell.

Telegony. The supposed influence of a former sire upon young born to a later sire by the same mother.

Type. The normal form of a species, which is regarded as typical.

Variation, Variability. The differing among themselves of individuals of the same species. When the extreme forms are connected by a complete series of intermediates, the variation is Continuous; when distinct forms occur, not connected by intermediates, it is Discontinuous.

Zygote. An individual produced by the union of two gametes.

INDEX

Ability, Inheritance of, 48

Abraxas, 121

Acquired Clmracters, 19, 24, 30,

45, 90-99, 136, 140 Albinism, 66, 71 Allelomorph, 56, 61, 71, 147 Amphidasys betularia, 89 Ancestral Heredity, Law of, 42 Andalusian Fowl, 69 Bateson, W., 19, 70, 76, 82, 125 Bee, 93, 129 BiFFEN, R. H. , 64 Biometric Methods, 9, 32-51 Boas, F., 96 (note) BovERi, T., 146 Brachydactyly, 106 Bkown-Sequard, 94 Canary, 124 Cat, 120 Cataract, 106

Chromosomes, 130, 140, 145-150 Colom'-blindness, 126, 127 Colour-inheritance, 71-83 Combs of Fowls, 66, 67 Correlation, 36

parental, 39, 103

Cunningham, J. T., 91 Currant Moth, 121 Darwin, 2, 8, 23, 24, 88, 136 Deafness, 103 Dihybridism, 62 Disease, Inheritance of, 102 Dominant Characters, 54 Drosophila, 126-128 Earwig, 17 Eldbbton, E. M., Hi

Environment, 24-20, 11*2-117

Epistatic, 73, 75

Eugenics, 51

Evolution, 2, 88, 136

Ewart, J. C, 98

Extracted Homozygote, 62, 86

Eye-colour, 66, 104, 109

Feeble-minded, 50

Fischer, E., 27, 93

Flatfish, 91

Flower-colour, 66, 74, 77, 79-83

Fluctuation, 22, 45

Fowls, 66, 67, 124

Galton, Sir F., 24, 30, 33, 41, 113

Gametes, 57 {see Germ-cells)

Gametic coupling, 125

Gemmules, 137

Germ-cells, 57, 137, 139, 145

Germinal Continuity, 138, 144

Germ -plasm, 20, 23-26, 139-144

Gregory, R. P., 82

Guinea-pig, 86, 94

Haemophilia, 126

Hair-colour, 66, 109

Hair-length, 66, 86

Herbst, C, 146

Heron, D., 103

Heterozygote, 59

Homostyle (Primula), 82

Homozygote, 61

Horns of Sheep, 84, 121

Hurst, C. C, 104, 109

Hybridisation, 53

Insanity, 102, 114

Instinct, 92

JOHANNSEN, W., 44, 87

160

INDEX

Kammekek, p., 06

Lamarck, 136

Leptinotarsa, 28

Lychnis, 66

Maize, 65, 66

Man, Heredity in, 101-117, 126

Marsh-marigold, 15

Maternal Impression, 99

Mendelian Heredity, 52-89

in Man, 102,

104-109 Mendel's Experiments, 53, 54 Mental and Moral Characters, 49 Mid-parent, 40 Mode, 10, 34 Morgan, T. H., 123 Mouse, 74 Mulatto, 110 Mutation, 22

Natural Selection, 8, 89, 136 Nettleship, E., 106, 108 Night-blindness, 106 Nucleus, 140, 145 Organic Stability, 24 Pangenesis, 137 Pearson, K., 32, 41, 115 (note) Peas, 54 Pigeons, 66 Primula, 66, 79, 81-83 Protoplasm, 3 PUNNETT, R. C, 70, 125 'Pure Lines,' 44, 87 Rabbit, 74, 79, 87 Rat, 72, 79, 96 (note) Recessive Characters, 56 Regression, 35 Retinitis pigmentosa, 107 Reversion, 74, 76 Rust in Wheat, 64

ScHtrsTEE, E., 49, 103

Sea-urchin eggs, 145, 146

Seed-colour, 66

Segregation, 58

Sex, 84, 105, 108, 119-134

Sheep, 84, 121

Silkworms, 94

Skew Curves, 15 Correlation, 39

Skin-colour, 110

Social Problems, 114

Standfuss, M., 27

Statistical Study of Variation, 32

Stature, 10, 34, 37

Sterility, 66, 112

Stocks, 66, 80

Sumner, F. B., 96 (note)

Sweet-pea, 54, 66, 76, 125

Telegony, 97

Temperature, effects of, 25, 27, 28

Tortoiseshell Cat, 84, 120

Tower, W. L., 28

Tuberculosis, 25

Twins, 30, 113, 129

Variation, 2, 5, 7

Causes of, 8, 23- 81, 142

Variation, Continuous and Dis- continuous, 9-12, 17-19, 22, 88

Variation, Curves illustrating, 11-17

Variation induced by crossing, 29

Vries, H. de, 19

Wallace, A. R., 8

Weismann, a., 20, 23, 138-144

Weldon, W. F. R., 33

Wheat, 64, 66

Wheldale, Muriel, 77

Yule, G. U., 42

Zygote, 58

CAMBRIDfiB : PRINTED BY JOHN CLAY, M.A, AT TUE UNlVEKSITy PKKBS

QH

D65 1912

Doncaster, Leonard

Heredity in the light of recent research

BioMed

PLEASE DO NOT REMOVE CARDS OR SLIPS FROM THIS POCKET

UNIVERSITY OF TORONTO LIBRARY