O? 1 Si LT) r=l CD m HEREDITY IN RELATION TO EVOLUTION AND ANIMAL BREEDING HEREDITY BY WILLIAM E. CASTLE PROFESSOR OF ZOOLOGY, HARVARD UNIVERSITY NEW YORK AND LONDON D. APPLE TON AND COMPANY 1911 COPYRIGHT, 1911, BY D. APPLETON AND COMPANY Published September, 1911 Printed in the United States of America PREFACE THIS little book is based on a course of eight lectures delivered in November and December, 1910, before the Lowell Institute, Boston, as well as . on a course of five lectures delivered before the Graduate School of Agriculture held under the auspices of the Association of Agricultural Colleges and Experiment Stations at Ames, Iowa, in July, 1910. The hope is entertained that it may be of service to students and that it will also interest the general reader. The writer wishes to express his gratitude to the Carnegie Institution of Washington for per- mission, in its preparation, to draw freely upon published and unpublished material derived from investigations aided by the Institution. Acknowledgment is also due to the following persons, or to their publishers, for permission to use figures from their publications, as indicated in the text : Prof. E. B. Wilson and The Mac- millan Co., Prof. H. S. Jennings and The Ameri- can Naturalist, Dr. W. B. Kirkham and The American Book Co. W. E. CASTLE JUNE, 1911 CONTENTS PAGE INTRODUCTION. — GENETICS, A NEW SCIENCE ... 1 CHAPTER I. — THE DUALITY OF INHERITANCE 6 II. — GERM- PLASM AND BODY, THEIR MUTUAL INDEPENDENCE 27 III. — MENDEL'S LAW OF HEREDITY 33 IV. — THE DETERMINATION OF DOMINANCE; HETEROZYGOUS CHARACTERS AND THEIR FIXATION; ATAVISM OR REVISION. ... 52 V. — EVOLUTION OF NEW RACES BY Loss OR GAIN OF CHARACTERS 72 VI. — EVOLUTION OF NEW RACES BY VARIATIONS IN THE POTENCY OF CHARACTERS .... 87 VII. — CAN MENDELIAN UNIT-CHARACTERS BE MODIFIED BY SELECTION? 106 VIII. — MENDELIAN INHERITANCE WITHOUT DOMI- NANCE, "BLENDING" INHERITANCE . . 128 IX. — THE EFFECTS OF INBREEDING 143 X. — HEREDITY AND SEX 153 INDEX 183 LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS FIG. PAGE 1. — Egg and sperm of the sea-urchin, Toxopneustes 9 2. — Fertilization of the egg of Nereis 12 3. — Egg of a mouse previous to maturation Facing 14 4. — Maturation and fertilization of the egg of a mouse . • Facing 14 5. — Diagrams showing the essential facts of chromosome reduction in the development of the sperm-cells 17 6. — An ordinary fern 21 7. — The prothallus of a fern 23 8. — Diagram showing the chromosome number in the spermatogenesis of ordinary animals and of the wasp 24 9. — Diagram showing the relation of the body to the germ-cells in heredity 29 10. — A young, black guinea-pig Facing 30 11. — An albino female guinea-pig Facing 30 12. — An albino male guinea-pig Facing 30 13. — Pictures of three living guinea-pigs and of the preserved skins of three others . . . Facing 32 14. — A black, female guinea-pig, and her young Facing 34 15. — An albino male guinea-pig Facing 34 16. — Two of the grown-up young of a black and of an albino guinea-pig Facing 34 ix x LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS FIG. PAGE 17. — A group of four young, produced by the animals shown in Fig. 16 ... .Facing 34 18. — Diagram to explain the result shown in Fig. 17 35 19. — A shortened condition of the skeleton, par- ticularly of the fingers Facing 36 20. — Radiograph of a hand similar to those shown in Fig. 19 Facing 38 21. — Diagram showing the descent, through five generations, of the condition shown in Figs. 19 and 20 40 22. — A smooth, dark guinea-pig ..... Facing 40 23. — A rough, white guinea-pig Facing 40 24. — A dark, rough guinea-pig Facing 40 25. — A smooth, white guinea-pig Facing 42 26. — A short-haired, pigmented guinea-pig Facing 42 27. — A long-haired albino guinea-pig . . . Facing 42 28. — Offspring produced by animals of the sorts shown in Figs. 26 and 27 ... .Facing 42 29. — A long-haired, pigmented guinea-pig, "Dutch- marked " with white Facing 42 30. — Diagram to explain the results of a cross between the sorts of guinea-pigs shown in Figs. 26 and 27 43 31. — Diagram showing the kinds and relative frequencies of the young to be expected in F2 from the crossing of animals shown in Figs. 26 and 27 46 32. — Along-haired, rough albino guinea- pig Facing 46 33. — Five new combinations of unit-characters obtained in generation F2, by crossing the animal shown in Fig. 32 with animals like that shown in Fig. 22 . , , . . , Facing 48 LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS xt FIG. PAGE 34. — Diagram to show the gametic combinations and segregations involved in a cross be- tween guinea-pigs differing in three unit- characters 49 35. — Diagram to show the gametic combination and recombinations which occur in the production and fixation of an atavistic coat- character in guinea-pigs 66 36. — An imperfectly rough guinea-pig . . .Facing 101 37. — A silvered guinea-pig Facing 101 38. — A. Front feet of an ordinary guinea-pig. B. Its hind feet D. Hind feet of a race four- toed on all the feet. C. Ordinary condition of the hind feet of young obtained by crossing B with D Facing 101 39. — Diagram showing variation in the color- pattern of hooded rats Facing 101 40. — Diagram showing the variations in size of eight different races of paramecium ... 112 41. — Chart showing effects of selection in eight successive generations upon the color- pattern of hooded rats 122 42. — Skulls of three rabbits Facing 128 43. — A long-haired, albino rabbit, having erect ears Facing 132 44. — A short-haired, sooty yellow rabbit, having lop ears Facing 132 45. — A short-haired, black rabbit, son of the rabbits shown in Figs. 43 and 44 . .Facing 132 46. — An F2 descendant of the rabbits shown in Figs. 44 and 45 .,,.,,., .Facing 132 xii LIST OF ILLUSTRATIONS FIG. PAGE 47. — Diagrams to show the number and size of the classes of individuals to be expected from a cross involving Mendelian segregation without dominance 135 48. — Photographs to show variation in ear length of two varieties of maize, of their Fx off- spring, and of their F2 offspring . .Facing 138 49. — Diagram of sex-determination in partheno- genesis 162 50. — Diagram of sex determination when the female is homozygous, the male heterozygous . . 167 51. — Diagram of sex-determination when the female is heterozygous, the male homozygous . . 170 52. — Diagram of sex-limited inheritance when the female is a heterozygote 173 53. — Diagram of sex-limited inheritance when the female is a homozygote, as in the red-eyed Drosophila 175 HEREDITY INTRODUCTION GENETICS, A NEW SCIENCE THE theory of organic evolution has prob- ably influenced more fields of human activity and influenced them more pro- foundly than has any other philosophic deduc- tion of ancient or modern times. By this theory philosophy, religion, and science have been rev- olutionized, while in the practical arts of educa- tion and agriculture, twin foundation stones of the state, man has been forced to adopt new methods of procedure or to justify the old ones in the light of a new principle. The evolutionary idea has forced man to con- sider the probable future of his own race on earth and to take measures to control that fu- ture, a matter he had previously left largely to fate. With a realization of the fact that or- 1 HEEEDITY ganisms change from age to age and that he himself is one of these changing organisms man has attained not only a new ground for humility of spirit but also a new ground for optimism and for belief in his own supreme importance, since the forces which control his destiny have been placed largely in his own hands. The existence of civilized man rests ultimately on his ability to produce from the earth in suf- ficient abundance cultivated plants and domes- ticated animals. City populations are apt to forget this fundamental fact and to regard with indifference bordering at times on scorn agri- cultural districts and their workers. But let the steady stream of supplies coming from the land to any large city be interrupted for only a few days by war, floods, a railroad strike, or any similar occurrence, and this sentiment vanishes instantly. Man to live must have food, and food comes chiefly from the land. A knowledge of how to produce useful animals and plants is therefore of prime importance. Civilization had its beginning in the attainment of such knowledge and is limited by it at the present day. If, therefore, this knowledge can 2 be increased, civilization may be advanced in a very direct and practical way. Before Darwin the practices of animal and plant breeders were largely empirical, based on unreasoned past ex- perience, just as was in antiquity the practice of metallurgy. Good plows and good swords were made long before a scientific knowledge of the metals was attained, but without that sci- entific knowledge the wonderful industrial de- velopment of this present age of steel would have been quite impossible. In a similar way, if not in like measure, we may reasonably hope for an advance in the productiveness of animal and plant breeding when the scientific principles which underlie these basic arts are better under- stood. Two practical problems present them- selves to the breeder: (1) how to make best use of existing breeds, and (2) how to create new and improved breeds better adapted to the con- ditions of present-day agriculture. We shall concern ourselves with the second of these only. The production of new and improved breeds of animals and plants is historically a matter about which we know scarcely more than about the production of new species in nature. Selec- 3 HEREDITY tion has been undoubtedly the efficient cause of change in both cases, but how and why applied and to what sort of material is as uncertain in one case as in the other. The few great men who have succeeded in producing by their individual efforts a new and more useful type of animal or plant have worked largely by empirical methods. They have produced a desired result but by methods which neither they nor any one else fully understood or could adequately explain. So there exists as yet no true science of breeding but only a highly developed art which was practiced as successfully by the ancient Egyptians, the Saracens, and the Romans as by us. The present, however, is an age of science; we are not satisfied with rule-of-thumb methods, we want to know the why as well as the how of our practical operations. Only such knowl- edge of the reasons for methods empirically successful can enable us to drop out of our practice all superfluous steps and roundabout methods and to proceed straight to the mark in the most direct way. The industrial his- tory of the last century is full of instances in 4 GENETICS, A NEW SCIENCE •which a knowledge of causes in relation to processes, i. e. a scientific knowledge, has shortened and improved practice in quite un- expected ways. So we may not doubt the ulti- mate value in practice of a science of breeding, if such a science can be created. A beginning has been made during the last ten years, starting with the rediscovery of Mendel's law of heredity in 1900. This book will be concerned largely with the operations of that law. CHAPTER I THE DUALITY OF INHERITANCE A the outset we may with profit inquire what is meant by heredity. When a child resembles a parent or grand- parent in some striking particular, we say it inherits such-and-such a characteristic from the parent or grandparent in question. By heredity, then, we mean organic resemblance based on descent. Resemblances due to heredity may exist even between individuals not related as ancestor and descendant, as for example between uncle and nephew. Here the resemblance rests on the fact that uncle and nephew are both descended from a common ancestor, and they resemble each other simply because they have both in- herited the same characteristic from that an- cestor. This form of inheritance is sometimes spoken of as collateral in distinction from direct 6 THE DUALITY OF INHEEITANCE inheritance. In all cases alike community of descent is the basis of resemblances which can be ascribed to heredity, whether direct or col- lateral. Mother and child, no less than uncle and nephew, resemble each other because they have received a common inheritance from a common ancestor. Three biological facts of fundamental im- portance to a right understanding of heredity were known imperfectly or not at all in the time of Darwin and Mendel. These are (1) the fertilization of the egg, (2) the maturation of the egg, which must precede its fertilization, and (3) the non-inheritance of " acquired " characters. These we may consider in order. Every new organism is derived from a pre- existing organism, so far as our present ex- perience goes. It may not have been so al- ways. Indeed, on the evolution theory, we must suppose that living matter originally arose from lifeless, inorganic matter. But if it did, this may have occurred, and probably did occur, under physical conditions quite different from those now existing. At the present time the most exhaustive researches 7 HEREDITY fail to reveal the occurrence of spontaneous generation, that is, the origin of living beings other than from pre-existing living beings. In asexual methods of reproduction a new individual arises out of a detached portion of the parent individual. Such methods of origin are varied and interesting, but do not concern us at present. In all the higher animals and plants a new individual arises, by what we call a sexual process, from the union of two minute bodies called the reproductive cells. They are an egg-cell furnished by the mother and a sperm-cell furnished by the father. There is a great difference in size between egg and sperm. The egg is many thousand times greater in bulk, as seen in Fig. 1, for example, yet the influence of each in heredity appears to be equal to that of the other. This fact shows unmistakably that the bulk of the reproductive cell is not significant in heredity. A large part of the relatively huge egg can have no part in heredity. It serves merely as food for the new organism, furnishing it with building material until such a time as it can begin to secure food for itself. The essential 8 THE DUALITY OF INHEKITANCE material, so far as heredity is concerned, is evidently found in egg and sperm alike. It is plainly small in amount and possibly con- sists merely in ferment-like bodies which ini- FIG. 1. — Egg and sperm (s) of the sea-urchin, Toxopneustes, both shown at the same enlargement. (After Wilson.) tiate certain metabolic processes in a suitable medium represented by the bulk of the egg. The amount of a ferment used in starting a chemical change bears no relation, as is well known, to the amount of the chemical change which it can bring about in a suitable medium. 9 HEKEDITY The equal share of egg and sperm in deter- mining the character of offspring is well shown in the following experiment. An albino guinea- pig is one which lacks in large measure the ability to form black pigment. Apparently it does not possess some ingredient or agency necessary for the production of pigment. Now, if an albino male guinea-pig, such as is shown in Fig. 15, be mated with a black female guinea- pig of pure race, such as is shown in Fig. 14, young are produced all of which are black, like the mother, none being albinos, like the father. Fig. 16 shows black offspring produced in this way. Exactly the same result is obtained from the reverse cross, that is, from mating an al- bino mother with a black sire. It makes no difference, then, whether the black parent be mother or father, its blackness regularly domi- nates over the whiteness of the albino parent, so that only black offspring result. This fact, which has been repeatedly confirmed, shows that the black character is transmitted as readily through the agency of the minute sperm-cell as through the enormously greater egg-cell. Let us now consider what happens when egg 10 THE DUALITY OF INHERITANCE and sperm unite, in what we call the fertiliza- tion of the egg. The egg is a rounded body incapable of motion, but the sperm is a minute thread-like body which moves like a tadpole by vibrations of its tail. In the case of most animals which live in the water, egg and sperm- cells are discharged into the water and there unite and develop into a new individual, but in the case of most land animals this union takes place within the body of the mother. We may consider an illustration of either sort. The fertilization of the egg of a marine worm, Nereis, is shown in Fig. 2. The thread- like sperm penetrates into the egg. Its en- larged head-end forms there a small nmtfear body, which increases in size until it equals that of the egg-nucleus, with which it then fuses. The egg next begins to divide up to form the different parts of a new worm-embryo. To each of these parts the nuclear material of egg and sperm is distributed equally. Since this development takes place wholly outside the body of either parent it is necessary that the egg contain enough food to last until the young worm can feed itself. This food material is 11 HEREDITY FIG. 2. — Fertilization of the egg of Nereis. A. The sperm has entered the egg and is forming a minute nucleus at o^. The egg-nucleus is breaking up preparatory to the first maturation division. B. The egg-nucleus is undergoing the first maturation division. Notice the con- spicuous rod-like chromosomes separating into two groups. The sperm-nucleus ( £ ) is now larger and lies deeper in the egg. C. A small polar-cell has been formed above by the first maturation division of the egg. A second division is in progress at the same point. The sperm-nucleus is now deep in the egg and is preceded by a double radiation (am- phiaster). D. Two polar-cells are fully formed. The ma- tured egg-nucleus is now fusing with the sperm-nucleus. An amphiaster indicates that division of the egg will soon take place. (After Wilson.) IS THE DUALITY OF INHEKITANCE represented in part by the conspicuous oil- drops seen in the egg (the heavy circles in Fig. 2). The egg of a mouse needs no such store of nourishment, since in common with the young of other mammals the mouse-embryo nourishes itself by osmosis from the body fluids of the mother. The mouse-egg is accordingly smaller. Stages in its fertilization are shown in Fig. 4. In A the sperm has already entered the egg. Eemnants of its thread-like tail may still be seen there. Nearby is seen a nuclear body derived from the sperm-head. Opposite is seen the nuclear body furnished by the egg itself. The two nuclear bodies fuse and their united substance is then distributed to all parts of the embryo-mouse, just as happens in the development of the worm, Nereis. There are reasons for thinking that the nuclear material is especially important in re- lation to heredity and that the equal share of the two parents in contributing it to the em- bryo is not without significance, for inheritance, as we have seen, is from both parents in equal measure. In cases where the inheritance from 13 HEREDITY each parent is different it can be shown that the offspring possess two inherited possi- bilities, though they may show but one. Thus in the case of a black guinea-pig, one of whose parents was white, the other black, it can be shown that the animal transmits both qualities (black and white) which it received from its respective parents, and transmits them in equal measure. For, if the cross-bred black animal be mated with a white one, half the offspring are black and half of them white. The cross- bred black animal inherited black from one parent, white from the other. It showed only the former, but on forming its reproductive cells it transmitted black to half of these, white to the other half. Hence the cross-bred black individual was a duality, containing two possi- bilities, black and white, but its reproductive cells were again single, containing either black or white, but not both. Now it has been shown in recent years that the nuclear material in the reproductive cells behaves exactly as do black and white in the cross just described. This nuclear material becomes doubled in amount at fertilization, U FIG. 3. — Egg of a mouse previous to maturation. (After Kirkham.) \ FIG. 4. — Maturation and fertilization of the egg of a mouse. A. The first maturation division in progress. B. The first polar-cell fully formed ; the second maturation division in progress. C. The second maturation division com- pleted ; the second polar-cell is the smaller one ; near it, in the egg, is the egg-nucleus, and at the left is the sperm- nucleus. D. A view similar to the last, but showing only one polar-cell, the second; note its twelve distinct chromosomes ; near the sperm-nucleus in the egg, at the left, is seen the thread-like remains of the sperm-tail. (After Kirkham.) THE DUALITY OF INHERITANCE equal contributions being made by egg and sperm. This double condition persists through- out the life of the new individual in all its parts and tissues. But if the individual forms eggs or sperm, these, before they can function in the production of a new individual, must undergo reduction to the single condition. This reduction process is called maturation; it is well illustrated in the case of the mouse- egg, whose fertilization has already been de- scribed. The large nucleus of the egg-cell, as it leaves the ovary, is either broken up or about to break up preparatory to a cell-division. The most conspicuous of the nuclear constituents are some dense, heavily staining bodies called chromosomes, about twenty-four in number. In Fig. 3 each of these is split in two, prepara- tory to the first maturation division. The egg now divides twice, both times very unequally (Fig. 4), forming thus two smaller cells called polar cells, or polar bodies. They take no part in the formation of the embryo. The chromo- somes left in the egg after these two divisions are only about half as numerous as before, or about twelve in number. These form the chro- 15 HEREDITY matin contribution of the egg to the production of a new individual. It is possible that other cell constituents undergo a similar reduction by half during maturation, but of this we have no present knowledge. The known fact of chromosome reduction, of course, favors the current interpretation that the chromosomes are bearers of heredity, though it by no means proves the correctness of that interpretation. In the egg of Nereis, as well as in that of the mouse, two matura- tion divisions precede the fertilization of the egg. See Fig. 2. In B the first maturation division is in progress; in C the second is in progress; and in D both polar cells are fully formed, while egg and sperm nuclei are unit- ing. Similar processes occur in eggs gener- ally, prior to their fertilization. Like changes occur also in the development of the sperm-cells. In Fig. 5 the original or unreduced condition of the chromosomes in a cell of the male sexual gland is shown (at A) as one of four chromosomes to a cell. After a series of changes involving as in the maturation of the egg two cell-divisions, we find (at H) that the 16 THE DUALITY OF INHERITANCE products of the original cell contain in each case two chromosomes, half the original number. FIG. 5. — Diagrams showing the essential facts of chromosome reduction in the development of the sperm-cells. (After Wilson.) These chromosomes make up the bulk of the head of the sperm which forms from each of 17 these cells, its tail being derived from other portions of the cell. It follows that not only eggs but also sperms, prior to their union in fertilization have passed into a reduced or single state as regards their chromatin constituents, whereas the fertilized egg, and the organism which develops from it, is in a double condition. It will be convenient to refer to the single condition as the N condi- tion, the double as the 2 N condition. From a wholly different source we have evidence strongly confirmatory of the conclu- sion that the fertilized egg contains a double dose of the essential nuclear material. By arti- ficial means it has been found possible to cause the development of an unfertilized egg. The means employed may be of several different sorts, such as stimulation with acids, alkalies, or solutions of altered density. In such ways the development has been brought about of the eggs of sea-urchins, star-fishes, worms, and mol- lusks, which normally require fertilization to make them develop. The sea-urchin egg has been made to develop more successfully than any other. This has 18 occurred even after the egg had undergone maturation, being reduced to the N condition. From the development of such reduced but unfertilized eggs fully normal sea-urchins have been obtained which even contain developed sexual glands. On the other hand it has been found possible to break the egg into fragments by shaking it, or cutting it into bits with fine knives or scissors. It has also been found possible to bring about the development of an egg fragment so obtained, — a fragment which contained no egg nucleus. This result has been attained by allowing a sperm to enter it and form there a nuclear body. No adult organism has yet been reared from such a fertilized egg-fragment, but so far as the de- velopment has been followed it progressed normally. There can accordingly be no doubt that the nuclear material of a sperm-cell has all the capabilities of that of an egg-cell and can in- deed replace it in development. Accordingly, when, as in normal fertilization, both an egg nucleus and a sperm nucleus are present in the cell, a double dose of the necessary nuclear 19 HEREDITY material is supplied. The second or extra dose is, however, not superfluous. It probably adds to the vigor of the organism produced, and in some cases at least, materially affects its form. For many animals and plants exist in two different conditions, in one of which the nu- clear components are simple, N, while in the other they are double, 2 N. Thus in bees, rotifers, and small Crustacea the egg may under certain conditions develop without being fertilized. If the egg develops before matura- tion is complete, that is in the 2 N condition, the animal produced is a female, like the mother which produced the egg. But if the egg undergoes reduction to the N condition before beginning its development, then it pro- duces a male individual, an organism, so far as reproduction is concerned, of lower meta- bolic activity. In many plants, too, individuals of N and of 2 N constitution occur, which differ markedly in appearance. Thus the ordinary fern-plant is a 2 N individual, but it never produces 2 N offspring. Fig. 6 shows an ordinary fern- plant, which produces spores on the under 20 THE DUALITY OF INHERITANCE FIG. 6. — An ordinary fern, which reproduces by asexual spores. The fern is shown reduced in size at 382; a portion of a frond seen from below and slightly enlarged, at 383; a cross-section of the same more highly magnified, at 384. Notice in 384 the sporangia, and in 385 one of these dis- charging spores. (After Wossidlo, from Coulter Barnes and Cowle's Textbook of Botany.) 3 21 HEREDITY surface of its fronds. Each of those spores is a reproductive cell which, like the mature eggs and sperm of animals, is in a reduced nuclear condition (N). These spores germi- nate, however, without uniting in pairs and form a plant different from the parent, just as the mature egg of a bee, if unfertilized, develops into an individual different from the parent, in that case a male. The plant which develops from the spore of a fern is small and inconspicuous and is known as a prothallus. See Fig. 7. It produces sexual cells (eggs and sperm ) which, uniting in pairs, form fern-plants, 2 N individuals. Thus there is a constant alter- nation of generations, fern-plants (2 N), which produce prothalli (N), and then these produce again fern-plants (2 N). The fact is worthy of note that in an animal or plant which is in the single or N condition, there occurs no chromatin reduction at the formation of reproductive cells. Its cells are already in the single condition, and they probably cannot be further reduced without destroying the organism. The 2 N fern-plant forms reproductive cells, its spores, which are 22 THE DUALITY OF INHEKITANCE in the reduced condition, N, and these germi- nate into the prothallus, which accordingly is FIG. 7. — The prothallus of a fern, which reproduces by sexual cells, eggs and sperm. The eggs are borne in the sac-like "archegonia," just below the notch in the figure. They, like the sperm -forming "antheridia," lie on the under sur- face of the flattened prothallus which is here viewed from below. Notice the root-hairs or rhizoids by which the plant feeds. Highly magnified. (After Coulter, Barnes, and Cowles.) N throughout. But when the prothallus forms reproductive cells, no reduction occurs. Its egg-cells and its sperm-cells in common with 23 HEREDITY all other cells of the prothallus are already in the reduced condition without any matura- tion divisions. The result of their union in pairs, at fertilization, is the formation of 2 N combinations that germinate into fern-plants. Similarly in the case of a male animal which FIG. 8. — Diagram showing the chromosome number in the spermatogenesis of ordinary animals (upper line) and of the wasp (lower line). has developed from a reduced but unfertilized egg, no reduction occurs at the formation of its sperm-cells. In an ordinary male animal, one which is in the double or 2 N state, the development of the sperms is attended by re- duction to the N condition. In this process there occur two cell-divisions producing from each initial cell four sperms. See Fig. 5, and 24 THE DUALITY OF INHEEITANCE Fig. 8, upper line. But in the male wasp, whose cells are in the N condition at the be- ginning, one of these divisions is so far sup- pressed that the resulting cell products are of very unequal size, and the smaller one contains no nuclear material. The other then gives rise to two sperm-cells, each possessing the origi- nal N nuclear condition, while the small non- nucleated cell degenerates. See Fig. 8, lower line. In conclusion, I wish to introduce two tech- nical terms, which it will be convenient for us to use in subsequent discussions. These are gamete and zygote. A reproductive cell (either egg or sperm) which is in the reduced condi- tion (N) ready for union in fertilization is called a gamete. The result of fertilization is a zygote, a joining together of two cells each in the N condition. The result is a new or- ganism, at first a single cell, in the 2 N condition. HEREDITY BIBLIOGRAPHY CASTLE, W. E. 1903. "The Heredity of Sex." Bull. Mus. Comp. Zool- ogy, 40, pp. 189-218. DELAGE, Y. 1898. "Embryons sans noyau maternel." Compte rendu, Academic des sciences, Paris, 127, pp. 528-531. 1909. "Le sexe chez les Oursins issus de parthe"- nogenese experimentale." Compte rendus, Academic des sciences, Paris, 148, pp. 453-455. KlRKHAM, W. B. 1907. Maturation of the Egg of the White Mouse." Trans. Conn. Acad. of Arts and Sciences, 13, pp. 65-87. LOEB, J. 1899. "On the Nature of the Process of Fertilization and the Artificial Production of Normal Larvae (Plutei) from the Unfertilized Eggs of the Sea-urchin." Amer. Journ. of Physiol, 3, pp. 135-138. LOTSY, J. P. 1905. "Die X-Generation und die 2 X-Generation." Biologisches Centralblatt, 25, pp. 97-117. MEVES, F., und DUESBERG, J. 1908. "Die Spermatozytenteilungen bei der Hornisse (Vespa crabo L.)." Arch. f. mik. Anat. u. Entwick., 71, pp. 571-587. WILSON, E. B. 1896. "The Cell in Development and Inheritance," 370 pp., illustrated. The Macmillan Co., New York. CHAPTER II GEBM-PLASM AND BODY, THEIR MUTUAL INDEPENDENCE IN the last chapter we discussed two bio- logical principles which, if clearly grasped, greatly simplify an understanding of the process of heredity. These are as follows: (1) A sexually produced individual arises from the union of two reproductive cells (or gametes), each of which contains, so far as heredity is concerned, a full material equip- ment for the production of a new individual. Accordingly, the newly produced individual is two-fold or duplex as concerns the material basis of heredity. (2) If the new individual becomes adult and forms gametes, the production of these will be attended by a reduction to the simplex or single condition as regards the material basis of heredity. 27 HEREDITY To these two principles we may now add a third, viz.: — (3) The individual consists of two distinct parts : first, its body destined to die and disintegrate after a certain length of time; and, secondly, the germ-cells con- tained within that body, capable of indefinite existence in a suitable medium. The fertilized egg or zygote begins its in- dependent existence by dividing into a number of cells. These become specialized to form the various parts and tissues of the body, muscle, bone, nerve, etc., and by becoming thus specialized they lose the power to produce any- thing but their own particular kind of special- ized tissue; they cannot reproduce the whole. This function is retained only by certain un- differentiated cells found in the reproductive glands and known as germ-cells. They are direct lineal descendants of the fertilized egg itself. If they are destroyed the individual loses the power of reproduction altogether. External influences which act upon the body may of course modify it profoundly, but such modifications are not transmitted through the gametes, because the gametes are not derived 28 GERM-PLASH AXD BODY from body-cells, but from germ-cells. This relationship first pointed out by Weismann may be expressed in a diagram, as in Fig. 9. Only such environmental influences as directly alter the character of the germ-cells will in any way influence the character of subsequent generations of individuals derived from those Line of succession. © Line of inheritance. C FIG. 9. — Diagram showing the relation of the body (•-* •*-*-> I £ ,ja 5 3 " BLENDING " INHERITANCE well as a sort which would produce only white seed, the progeny namely of the expected white seed of F2, but as that was not obtained, the all-white plant of F3 could not be obtained either. The expected proportions of the sev- eral classes in F3 are given for comparison with those actually obtained. The agreement be- tween expected and observed is so good as to make it seem highly probable that Nilsson- Ehle's explanation is correct. Corroborative evidence in the case of maize has been obtained by Dr. E. M. East (Am. Naturalist, Feb., 1910). This work introduces us to a new principle which may have important theoretical conse- quences. If a character ordinarily represented by a single unit in the germ-plasm may become represented by two or more such units identi- cal in character, then we may expect it to domi- nate more persistently in crosses, fewer reces- sives being formed in F2 and subsequent gen- erations. Further, if duplication of a unit tends to increase its intensity, as seems probable, then we have in this process a possible expla- nation of quantitative variation in characters which are non-Mendelian, or at any rate do not 10 133 HEREDITY conform with a simple Mendelian system. Con- sider, for example, the matter of size and skeletal proportions in rabbits. It is perfectly clear from the experiments described that in such cases no dominance occurs, and also that no segregation of a simple Mendelian character takes place, but it is not certain that the ob- served facts may not be explained by the com- bined action of several similar but independent factors, the new principle which Nilsson-Ehle has brought to our attention. Let us apply such a hypothesis to the case in hand. Suppose a cross be made involving ear- lengths of approximately 4 and 8 inches respec- tively, as in one of the crosses made. The Ft young are found to have ears about 6 inches long, the mean of the parental conditions, and the F2 young vary about the same mean con- dition. If a single Mendelian unit-character made the difference between a 4 inch and an 8 inch ear, the F2 young should be of three classes as follows: Classes 4 in. 6 in. 8 in. Frequencies 121 134 " BLENDING » INHERITANCE (Compare Fig. 47, bottom left.) The grand- parental conditions should in this case reappear .1 I. .ill. i i FIG. 47. — Diagrams to show the number and size of the classes of individuals to be expected from a cross involving Mende- lian segregation without dominance. One Mendelian unit involved, bottom left; two units, middle left; three units, top left; four units, right. in half the young. This clearly does not oc- cur in the rabbit experiment. But if two unit- 135 HEREDITY characters were involved, Fj would be un- changed, all 6 inches, yet the F2 classes would be more numerous, viz., 4, 5, 6, 7, and 8 inches, and their relative frequencies as shown by the height of the columns in Fig. 47, middle left, 1, 4, 6, 4, 1. The grandparental states would now re- appear in i/s °f the F2 young, while % would be intermediate. It is certain, however, that in rab- bits the grandparental conditions, if they re- appear at all, do not reappear with any such frequency as this. If three independent size-factors were in- volved in the cross, the F1 individuals should all fall in the same middle group, as before, viz. 6 inches, but the F2 classes should number seven, and their relative frequencies would be as shown in Fig. 47, top left. For 4 independ- ent size-factors, the F2 classes would be more numerous still, viz., 9 (Fig. 47, right), and the extreme ear-size of either grandparent would be expected to reappear in only one out of 256 offspring, while considerably more than half of them would fall within the closely inter- mediate classes included between 5^ and 6l/o inches, the three middle classes of the diagram. 136 With six size-characters, the extreme size of a grandparent would reappear no oftener than once in 4000 times, while with a dozen such independent characters it would recur only once in some 17,000,000 times. It would be remarkable if under such conditions the ex- treme size were ever recovered from an ordi- nary cross. There is one means by which we can deter- mine with certainty whether in a particular case of seemingly blending inheritance segre- gation does or does not occur, namely, by com- paring the variability of the Fl and the F2 generations. If segregation does not occur, F2 should be no more variable than Fx, whereas if segregation does occur, F2 should be more variable. For, in a segregating system, the F± individuals should all fall in a middle, inter- mediate group, but the F2 individuals should be distributed also in classes more remote from a strictly intermediate position, that is, they should be more variable. But, in a non-segre- gating system, Ft and F2 individuals alike should fall in the same intermediate group, that is, they should have the same variability. 137 HEREDITY The matter should be easy of determination by observation of considerable numbers of i\ and F2 offspring. Investigations are now in progress to test this matter. My colleague, Dr. East, has found clear evi- dence that, in maize, size-characters, although they give a blending result in Fj, nevertheless give segregation in F2. The character to be con- sidered relates to length of ear in corn. A single illustration will suffice. The variation in two pure varieties is shown in the two upper rows of Fig. 48. The " Length " of each class is given in centimetres, its frequency just below at ' ' No. Var., ' ' abbreviation for number of variates. The variation in the Fx offspring obtained by cross- ing the two pure varieties is shown in the third row, and that of the F2 offspring in the lowest row. Note that the variability in the Fx gen- eration is not increased; its range is interme- diate between the range in the parental varie- ties. In the F2 generation, however, the varia- bility is so increased that it includes almost the entire range of both parental varieties, to- gether with the intervening region. In the light of this evidence it is clear that 138 3 :r ». AS 16 ,'f 10 7 1 . X-' I 10 II It *7 73 6S (S 39 2S (f 9 f . FIG. 48. — Photographs to show variation in ear length of two varie- ties of maize (upper row), of their F! offspring (second row), and of their F2 offspring (third row). (After East.) " BLENDING " INHERITANCE in maize, seemingly blending is really segre- gating inheritance, but with entire absence of dominance, and it seems probable that the same will be found to be true among rabbits and other mammals; failure to observe it hitherto is probably due to the fact that the factors concerned are numerous. For the greater the number of factors concerned, the more nearly will the result obtained approximate a com- plete and permanent blend. As the number of factors approaches infinity, the result will become identical with a permanent blend. Theoretically it is important to know whether segregating units are involved in inheritance which we call blending; practically it does not matter much, since if these units are only as numerous as six or eight it will be practically impossible to undo the effects of a cross and to recover again the conditions obtaining previous to the cross. The great majority of the offspring both in the first and in subsequent generations following the cross will be strictly intermediate between the conditions crossed whether several units, an infinite number of units, or no units at all are involved. 139 HEREDITY A practical question of some importance is how to manipulate simultaneously blending (or seemingly blending) and Mendelian inheritance. This must be by a system of line-breeding in alternate generations, not in successive genera- tions. To test the practicability of this matter I several years ago set myself the task of com- bining in one race the large size of some lop- eared, yellow rabbits which I had, with the albino character of some small white rabbits of common race. A first cross produced gray rabbits of intermediate size, but no white ones. On inbreeding the gray animals, there were obtained in F2 white young of intermediate size. These were now crossed again with the original yellow stock, and again colored young were obtained, but now with % of the desired increase in size. These bred inter se again produced albinos, this time of the % size. A third cross with the original large stock brought the size up to % of that desired, and combined it in F2 with the desired albinism. Having satisfied myself of the correctness of the method, the experiment was now discontinued. By further crosses, especially with a fresh lop- 140 " BLENDING " INHEEITANCE eared stock, to avoid ill-effects of inbreeding, the size could have been still further increased, with judicious selection doubtless up to the ex- treme size of colored lop-eared rabbits. The general conclusion to be drawn is that in attempting to combine in one race by cross- breeding characters which exist separately in different races, one should first inquire very carefully how each character, in which the races differ, behaves in transmission, for on the an- swer to this question should depend the mode of procedure to be chosen. If simple Mendelian characters only are con- cerned, nothing is required but to cross the two races and select from the second generation offspring the desired combination. If blending characters only are concerned and F± yields the desired blend, this is secure without fur- ther procedure, except possibly selection to re- duce its variability; but if the desired blend is not yet secured, further back-crossing with one race or the other may be necessary. If, finally, both blending and Mendelian characters are simultaneously involved in a cross, then the method of combined line-breeding and selec- 141 HEREDITY tion in alternate generations, already described, should be adopted. BIBLIOGRAPHY CASTLE, W. E. 1909. (See Bibliography to Chapter V.) EAST, E. M. 1910. "A Mendelian Interpretation of Variation that is Apparently Continuous." American Naturalist, 44, pp. 65-82. EMERSON, R. A. 1910. " The Inheritance of Sizes and Shapes in Plants." American Naturalist, 44, pp. 739-746. NILSSON-EHLE, H. 1909. " Kreuzungsuntersuchungen an Hafer und Weizen." Lunds Universitets Arsskrift, 5, No. 2, 122 pp. CHAPTER IX THE EFFECTS OP INBREEDING WHAT is the probable source of the evil effects which have been fre- quently observed to follow in- breeding? By inbreeding we mean the mating of closely \ related individuals. As there are different de- grees of relationship between individuals, so there are different degrees of inbreeding. The closest possible inbreeding occurs among plants in what we call self-pollination, in which the egg-cells of the plant are fertilized by pollen- cells produced by the same individual. A simi- lar phenomenon occurs among some of the lower animals, notably among parasites. But in all the higher animals, including the domes- ticated ones, such a thing is impossible because of the separateness of the sexes. For here no individual produces both eggs and sperm. The 143 HEKEDITY nearest possible approach to self-pollination is in such cases the mating of brother with sister, or of parent with child. But this is less close inbreeding than occurs in self-pollination, for the individuals mated are not in this case iden- tical zygotes, though they may be similar ones. It has long been known that in many plants self-pollination is habitual and is attended by no recognizable ill-effects. This fortunate cir- cumstance allowed Mendel to make his remark- able discovery by studies of garden-peas, in which the flower is regularly self-fertilized, and never opens at all unless made to do so by some outside agency. Self-pollination is also the rule in wheat, oats, and the majority of the other cereal crops, the most important econom- ically of cultivated plants. Crossing can in such plants be brought about only by a difficult technical process, so habitual is self-pollina- tion. And crossing, too, in such plants is of no particular benefit, unless by it one desires to secure new combinations of unit-characters. In maize, or Indian corn, however, among the cereals, the case is quite different. Here enforced self-pollination results in small un- 144 EFFECTS OF INBREEDING productive plants, lacking in vigor. But racial vigor is fully restored by a cross between two depauperate unproductive individuals obtained by self-fertilization, as has recently been shown by Shull. This result is entirely in harmony with those obtained by Darwin, who showed by long-continued and elaborate experiments that while some plants do not habitually cross and are not even benefited by crossing, yet in many other plants crossing results in more vigorous and more productive offspring; that further, the advantage of crossing in such cases has resulted in the evolution in many plants of floral structures, which insure crossing through the agency of insects or of the wind. In animals the facts as regards close fer- tilization are similar to those just described for plants. Some animals seem to be indiffer- ent to close breeding, others will not tolerate it. Some hermaphroditic animals (those which produce both eggs and sperm) are regularly self -fertilized. Such is the case, for example, with many parasitic flat- worms. In other cases self-fertilization is disadvantageous. One such case I was able to point out some fifteen years 145 ago, in the case of a sea-squirt or tunicate, Ciona. The same individual of Ciona produces and discharges simultaneously both eggs and sperm, yet the eggs are rarely self-fertilized, for if self-fertilization is enforced by isolation of an individual, or if self-fertilization is brought about artificially by removing the eggs and sperm from the body of the parent and mixing them in sea-water, very few of the eggs develop, — less than 10%. But if the eggs of one individual be mingled with the sperm of any other individual whatever, prac- tically all of the eggs are fertilized and develop. In the great majority of animals, as in many plants, self-fertilization is rendered wholly im- possible by separation of the sexes. The same individual does not produce both eggs and sperm, but only one sort of sexual product. But among sexually separate animals the same degree of inbreeding varies in its effects. The closest degree, mating of brother with sister, has in some cases no observable ill-effects. Thus, in the case of a small fly, Drosophila, my pupils and I bred brother with sister for 146 EFFECTS OF INBREEDING fifty-nine generations in succession without ob- taining a diminution in either the vigor or the fecundity of the race, which could with cer- tainty be attributed to that cause. A slight diminution was observed in some cases, but this was wholly obviated when parents were chosen from the more vigorous broods in each generation. Nevertheless crossing of two in- bred strains of Drosophila, both of which were doing well under inbreeding, produced off- spring superior in productiveness to either inbred strain. Even in this case, therefore, though inbreeding is tolerated, cross-breeding has advantages. In the case of many domesticated animals, it is the opinion of experienced breeders, sup- ported by such scientific observations as we possess, that decidedly bad effects follow con- tinuous inbreeding. Bos ( '94) practiced con- tinuous inbreeding with a family of rats for six years. No ill-effects were observed during the first half of the experiment, but after that a rapid decline occurred in the vigor and fer- tility of the race. The average-sized litter in the first half of the experiment was about 7.5, 147 HEREDITY but in the last year of the experiment it had fallen to 3.2, and many pairs were found to be completely sterile. Diminution in size also attended the inbreeding, at the end amounting in the case of males to between 8 and 20 %. Experiments made by Weismann confirm those of Bos as regards the falling off in fer- tility due to inbreeding. For eight years Weis- mann bred a colony of mice started from nine individuals, — six females and three males. The experiment covered 29 generations. In the first 10 generations the average number of young to a litter was 6.1; in the next 10 gen- erations, it was 5.6; and in the last 9 genera- tions, it had fallen to 4.2. But sweeping generalizations cannot be drawn from these cases. Each species of animal must probably be tested for itself before we shall know what the exact effects of inbreeding are in that case. In guinea-pigs, a polydactylous race built up by the closest inbreeding out of individuals all descended from one and the same individual has now been in existence for ten years. It consists of one of the largest and most vigor- ous strains of guinea-pigs that I have ever 148 seen, and has shown no indications of dimin- ished fertility. In the production of pure breeds of sheep, cattle, hogs, and horses inbreeding has fre- quently been practiced extensively, and where in such cases selection has been made of the more vigorous offspring as parents, it is doubt- ful whether any diminution in size, vigor, or fertility has resulted. Nevertheless it very frequently happens that when two pure breeds are crossed, the offspring surpass either pure race in size and vigor. This is the reason for much cross-breeding in economic practice, the object of which is not the production of a new breed, but the production for the market of an animal maturing quickly or of superior size and vigor. The inbreeding practiced in form- ing a pure breed has not of necessity dimin- ished vigor, but a cross does temporarily (that is in the Fj generation) increase vigor above the normal. Now why should inbreeding un- attended by selection decrease vigor, and cross- breeding increase it? We know that inbreed- ing tends to the production of homozygous conditions, whereas cross-breeding tends to 11 149 HEREDITY produce heterozygous conditions. Under self- pollination for 1 generation following a cross, half the offspring become homozygous; after 2 generations, % of the offspring are homo- zygous ; after 3 generations % are homozygous, and so on. So if the closest inbreeding is practiced there is a speedy return to homo- zygous, pure racial conditions. We know fur- ther that in some cases at least heterozygotes are more vigorous than homozygotes. The heterozygous yellow mouse is a vigorous lively animal; the homozygous yellow mouse is so feeble that it perishes as soon as produced, never attaining maturity. Cross-breeding has, then, the same advantage over close-breeding that fertilization has over parthenogenesis. It brings together differentiated gametes, which, reacting on each other, produce greater meta- bolic activity. Inbreeding, also, by its tendency to secure homozygous combinations, tends to bring to the surface latent or hidden recessive charac- ters. If these are in nature defects or weak- nesses of the organism, such as albinism and feeble-mindedness in man, then inbreeding is 150 EFFECTS OF INBREEDING distinctly bad. Existing legislation against the marriage of near-of-Mn is, therefore, on the whole, biologically justified. On the other hand, continual crossing only tends to hide inherent defects, not to exterminate them; and inbreeding only tends to bring them to the surface, not to create them. We may not, therefore, lightly ascribe to inbreeding or in- termarriage the creation of bad racial traits, but only their manifestation. Further, any racial stock which maintains a high standard of excellence under inbreeding is certainly one of great vigor, and free from inherent defects. The animal breeder is therefore amply jus- tified in doing what human society at present is probably not warranted in doing, — viz. in practicing close inbreeding in building up families of superior excellence and then keep- ing these pure, while using them in crosses with other stocks. For an animal of such a superior race should have only vigorous, strong offspring if mated with a healthy individual of any family whatever, within the same spe- cies. For this reason the production of " thoroughbred ': animals and their use in 151 HEREDITY crosses is both scientifically correct and com- mercially remunerative. BIBLIOGRAPHY BOS, RlTZEMA. 1894. "Untersuchungen ueber die Folgen der Zucht in engster Blutverwandtschaft." Biol. Centrbl., 14, pp. 75- 81. CASTLE, W. E., CARPENTER, F. W., CLARK, A. H., MAST, S. O., and BARROWS, W. M. "The Effects of Inbreeding, Cross-breeding and Selec- tion upon the Fertility and Variability of Drosophila." Proc. Amer. Acad. Arts and Sci., 41, pp. 731-786. GUAITA, G. VON. 1898. "Versuche mit Kreuzungen von verschiedenen Rassen des Hausmaus." Ber. naturf. Gesellsch. zu Freiburg, 10, pp. 317-332. [Contains observations of Weismann.] CHAPTER X HEEEDITY AND SEX THE value of a domesticated animal often depends in considerable measure on its sex. Therefore, if a means could be devised for controlling the sex of offspring, it would be of great economic value to the breeder. Endless attempts have been made to do this, and occasionally a claim of success has been made, but none of these claims has withstood the test of critical analysis or experiment. The hypotheses advanced to explain how sex may be controlled have been of the most varied character. In some the determination of sex has been supposed to inhere in the nature of the parents, in others it is referred to condi- tions of the gametes themselves. Relative age or vigor of the parents have been supposed to influence sex in various ways. The same idea has been advanced regarding 153 HEREDITY the gametes themselves, it being supposed that early or late fertilization of the egg might influence its sex. Experimental evidence, how- ever, as to these several hypotheses is wholly negative, when one eliminates other possible factors from the experiment. Everything points to the conclusion that sex rests in the last analysis upon gametic differentiation, just as the color of a guinea-pig in a mixed race of blacks and whites depends upon whether the gametes which unite to produce it carry black or white. As the heterozygous black guinea- pig forms black-producing and white-producing gametes in equal numbers, so there is reason to think male-producing and female-producing gametes are formed in equal numbers by the parent, in many cases at least. But is it not possible that there may exist individuals which produce the two sorts of gametes in unequal numbers, and so would have a tendency to produce more offspring of one sex than of the other? Perhaps so, though we have no evi- dence that such a condition, if it does exist, is transmitted from one generation to another. On this point I made experimental observa- 154 HEREDITY AND SEX tions upon guinea-pigs extending over a series of years. Oftentimes I found an individual that produced more offspring of one sex than of the other, but this was probably due merely to chance deviations from equality. I could get no evidence that the condition was inher- ited, though the experiment was continued through as many as seven generations, includ- ing several hundred offspring. The essential difference between a female and a male individual is that one produces eggs, the other sperm. All other differences are secondary and dependent largely upon the differences mentioned. If in the higher ani- mals (birds and mammals) the sex-glands (i. e. the egg-producing and sperm-producing tis- sues) are removed from the body, the super- ficial differences between the sexes largely dis- appear. In insects, however, the secondary sex-characters seem to be for the most part uninfluenced by presence or absence of the sex-glands. Their differentiation occurs in- dependently though simultaneously with that of the sex-glands. The egg or larger gamete (the so-called 155 HEREDITY macro-gamete) in all animals is non-motile and contains a relatively large amount of reserve- food material for the maintenance of the de- veloping embryo. This reserve-food material it is the function of the mother to supply. In the case of some animals, for example flat- worms and mollusks, the food-supply of the embryo is not stored in the egg-cell itself, but in other cells associated with it, and which break down and supply nourishment to the developing embryo derived from the fertilized egg. Again, as in the mammals, the embryo may derive its nourishment largely from the maternal tissues, the embryo remaining like a parasite within the maternal body during its growth, feeding by absorption. But in all cases alike the mother supplies the larger gamete and the food-material necessary to carry the zygote through its embryonic stages. The father, on the other hand, furnishes the bare hereditary equipment of a gamete, with the motor apparatus necessary to bring it into contact with the egg-cell, but without food for the developing embryo produced by fertiliza- tion. The gamete furnished by the father is 156 HEREDITY AND SEX therefore the smaller gamete, the so-called micro-gamete. From the standpoint of metabolism, the female is the more advanced condition; the female performs the larger function, doing all that the male does in furnishing the material basis of heredity (a gamete), and in addition supplying food for the embryo. As regards the reproductive function, the female is the equivalent of the male organism, plus an ad- ditional function, — that of supplying the em- bryo with food. When we come to consider the structural basis of sex, we find reasons for thinking that here, too, the female individual is the equivalent of the male plus an addi- tional element. The conclusion has very natu- rally been drawn that if a means could be devised for increasing the nourishment of the egg or embryo, its development into a female should be thereby insured, while the reverse treatment should lead to the production of a male. But in practice this a priori expecta- tion is not fulfilled. Better nourishment of the mother may lead to the production of more eggs, but not of more female offspring, as has 15? HEREDITY been repeatedly demonstrated by experiment. Also poor nutrition of the mother may diminish the number of eggs which she liberates, but will not increase the proportion of males among the offspring produced. An excellent summary of evidence on this point was made by Cuenot in 1900. Attempts to influence the sex of an embryo or larva by altered nutrition of the embryo or larva itself have proved equally futile. Practically the only experimental evidence of value in favor of this idea has been derived from the study of insects, and this is capable of explanation on quite different grounds from those which first suggest themselves. It has sometimes been observed, as by Mary Treat for example, that a lot of insects poorly fed produce an excess of males. In such lots, however, the mortality is commonly high, and more females die than males, because the female is usually larger and requires more food to complete its development. The fallacy in concluding from such evidence that scanty nutrition causes individuals which would otherwise become females to develop into males was indicated years ago by Eiley. 158 HEREDITY AND SEX Nevertheless an argument for the artificial con- trol of sex based on such evidence is from time to time brought forward, as, for example, a few years since by Schenk. The latest advocate of sex-control by artificial means is an Italian, Russo (1909). He claims in the case of rabbits that by feeding the mother on lecithin or by injections of lecithin, the proportion of female births may be increased. His evidence in sup- port of this claim is, however, wholly inade- quate, and two independent repetitions of his experiments, made by Basile in Italy and by Punnett in England, have given entirely nega- tive results. An alternative hypothesis concerning the de- termination of sex has been steadily gaining ground during the last ten years, that sex has its beginning in gametic differentiation and is finally determined beyond recall in the ferti- lized egg by the nature of the uniting gametes. Instructive in this connection is a study of parthenogenesis, — reproduction by unfertilized eggs. But before entering upon this, it may be well to review briefly the changes which regularly take place in the egg which is to be 159 HEREDITY fertilized, and compare with this the changes which occur in eggs not to Be fertilized. In each cell of the ordinary animal there occurs a characteristic number of bodies called chromosomes. We do not know that they are any more important than other cell constitu- ents, but we know their history better. These are contained in the nucleus of the cell, and at the time of nuclear division they are found at the equator of the division spindle. For ex- ample, in the egg of the mouse (Fig. 4, A), the nucleus is seen to be in the spindle stage, and its chromosomes are gathered together at the equator of the spindle. There each of them regularly splits in two, and one derivative goes to either end of the spindle, and so into one of the daughter-nuclei. Thus each new nucleus has, as a rule, the same chromosome composi- tion as the nucleus from which it was derived. But the egg which is to be fertilized under- goes two nuclear divisions in succession, in only one of which do the chromosomes split (see Fig. 4, A-D). In the other division the chromo- somes separate into two groups without split- ting, and each group goes into a different cell 160 HEREDITY AND SEX product. Consequently, in each of these prod- ucts the number of chromosomes is reduced to half what it is in the cells of the parental body. Thus in the egg of the mouse, by maturation, the number of chromosomes becomes reduced from about twenty-four to about twelve. Similar changes occur in the developing sperm-cell (see Fig. 5). Starting with the double or 2 N chromosome number, there are formed by two nuclear divisions, with only one splitting of chromosomes, four cells, each with the reduced or simplex number of chromosomes, N. Consequently, when the sperm enters the egg at fertilization it brings in a group of N chromosomes (in the mouse apparently twelve), which, added to the egg-contribution of N chromosomes, brings the number in the new organism again up to 2 N (in the mouse twenty- four). Now, as regards the maturation of partheno- genetic eggs, those which are to develop with- out having been fertilized, three categories of cases deserve separate discussion. The simplest of these in many respects is found among the social hymenoptera (ants, bees, and wasps). 161 HEEEDITY See Fig. 49, left column. The eggs are, so far as we can discover, all of a single type. They BEE KOTIFER APHID ? \/ ^ — " / v, — /» \ in \ FIG. 49. — Diagram of sex-determination in parthenogenesis. First row, nuclear condition of the parthenogenetic mother; second row, of her eggs when they develop without reduction, after forming a single polar cell; third row, condition of the eggs after complete maturation — the fertilized egg in each case produces a male; fourth row, nuclear condition of the fertilized egg, always a female. undergo maturation in the manner already de- scribed, the chromosomes being reduced to the 162 HEREDITY AND SEX N or simplex number. The eggs of most ani- mals, after they have undergone reduction, are incapable of development unless fertilized, but those of the hymenoptera may develop either fertilized or unfertilized. In the former case a female is produced, in the latter a male. The simplex, or N condition is in this case the male, the duplex or 2 N condition is the female, natu- rally the one of higher metabolic activity, the one which forms the macro-gametes. In an earlier chapter I explained how the development of the sperm-cells in a male having the reduced or simplex number of chromosomes differs from that in the ordinary male. Refer- ence to Fig. 8 may help to recall this. The cells of the male are in this case already in the reduced or simplex condition, N. In the pro- duction of the sperms the reducing division is omitted so far as nuclear components are con- cerned, so that each sperm formed contains the full simplex chromosome number, N. If it were less, the gamete formed would perhaps not be capable of transmitting all the hereditary char- acteristics of an individual. A second category of cases (Fig. 49, middle 163 HEREDITY column) is represented by such simple aquatic organisms as rotifers and small Crustacea, like Daphnia. In these parthenogenesis occurs ex- clusively, when the food supply is very abun- dant and conditions otherwise favorable, whereas reproduction by fertilized eggs occurs only when external conditions, including food-supply, are not good. Under favorable conditions only female offspring are produced. The conclusion has naturally but erroneously been drawn that good nutrition in itself favors the production of females in animals generally, which is not true. The egg produced by Daphnia, or by a rotifer, under optimum conditions does not un- dergo reduction (see Fig. 49, second row). It remains in the 2 N condition, forming but a single polar cell. It is therefore unprepared for fertilization, and in fact it is not fertilized. Its sex is like that of the animal which formed it, female. Under unfavorable conditions, how- ever, the eggs of the rotifer and of Daphnia do not begin development until they have un- dergone maturation. They are also of two sizes (Fig. 49, third row), — small eggs, which develop without fertilization and which form 164 HEREDITY AND SEX males, and large eggs, which require fer- tilization, and which form females. In this category of cases, as in that of the hymenop- tera, the egg which develops in the 2 N condi- tion, either from failure of reduction to occur in maturation or from fertilization following reduction, forms a female; whereas the egg which develops in the N condition forms a male. In a third category of cases there is a quan- titative difference in chromatin between male and female, just as in the foregoing cases, but this does not amount to a whole set of chromo- somes, N, but to only a partial set, one or two chromosomes (see Fig. 49, right column). This category of cases occurs in plant-lice (aphids and phylloxerans) ; evidence of its existence rests chiefly on recent observations made by von Baehr and Morgan. Females are formed by parthenogenesis without reduction, occurring under favorable conditions, just as in the case of rotifers. Females are also formed by fer- tilization following reduction under unfavor- able conditions, just as in rotifers. In both cases the female is 2 N. Males arise only by 12 165 HEEEDITY parthenogenesis under unfavorable conditions, just as in rotifers, but the reduction which occurs before development begins is partial only. A whole set, N, of chromosomes is not eliminated in maturation, but only 1 or 2 chro- mosomes. Hence the male condition here is 2 N — 1 or — 2. The condition of the gametes formed, however, is N in both sexes. In spermatogenesis, division of the germ-cells takes place into N and N -- 1 daughter cells, but the latter degenerate (like the non-nucleated cells of the bee and wasp), and only the former produce spermatozoa. Hence in fertilization only 2 N zygotes are produced, which are in- variably female. Summarizing the three categories described, we may say that in all known cases of par- thenogenesis, the female is in the duplex, 2 N condition, the male in the simplex (N) or par- tially duplex condition (2N--1, or 2 N — 2). The female in all cases has the greater chro- matin content. In a great many insects and other arthro- pods, which are not parthenogenetic, it is known that, although the male, like the female, 166 HEEEDITY AND SEX develops only from a fertilized egg, neverthe- less the male possesses fewer chromosomes than FIG. 50. — Diagram of sex-determination when the female is homozygous, the male heterozygous. the female. In such cases the female forms, as in cases of parthenogenesis, only N gametes, but the male forms gametes of two sorts, N and 167 HEREDITY N — 1 or N — 2 (see Fig. 50). In consequence zygotes of two sorts result, — those which are 2 N, female, and those which are 2 N -- 1 or 2 N — 2, male. Thus in the squash-bug, Anasa- tristis, according to Wilson, the mature egg contains 11 chromosomes, the spermatozoa either 10 or 11 chromosomes, the two sorts being equally numerous. Egg 11 + sperm 11 produces a zygote 22 (2N), a female; Egg 11+ " 10 " " " 21(2N-l),amale. N in this species = 11 ; 2 N = 22, the female ; 2 N — 1 == 21, the male. Males and females are therefore approximately equal in number, as in most animals where the two sexes are not sub- ject to unequal mortality. In the Mendelian sense the female is in such cases a homozygote, the male a heterozygote. The sex of an indi- vidual in such cases depends upon which sort of a sperm chances to enter the egg. But the experimental evidence indicates that both as regards sex and as regards heritable characters correlated with sex, these relations may in some cases be reversed, the female being heterozygous, the male homozy- 168 HEREDITY AND SEX gous. In such cases there is reason to think that structurally the male is 2 N but the female 2 N -J-. That is, the female is still the equiva- lent of the male plus some additional element and function. A structural basis in the chromo- somes for such a condition has been described by Baltzer in the case of the sea-urchin. He found the regular duplex number of chromo- somes in the male; but in the female, while the number was the same, one of the chromo- somes was larger than its mate, having an extra or odd element attached to it. In such a case the gametes formed by the male would all be N, but those formed by the female would be of two sorts equally numerous, viz. N and N -f- (see Fig. 51). Egg N fertilized by sperm N would produce a zygote 2 N, a male ; egg N -(- fertilized by sperm N would produce a zygote 2 N +, a female. Hence here, as in other ani- mals, the sexes would be approximately equal, but the sex of a particular individual would depend upon which sort of egg gave rise to it. Upon the existence, as in the foregoing cases, of an unpaired or odd structural element in the egg, may perhaps depend the explanation 169 HEEEDITY of a curious sort of heredity known as sex- limited heredity. FIG. 51. — Diagram of sex-determination when the female is heterozygous, the male homozygous. Every one who knows anything about poultry is acquainted with the popular American breed known as the barred Plymouth Bock. In this 170 HEEEDITY AND SEX breed the feathers are marked with alternate bars of darker and lighter black. Pure barred Eocks breed true, but when crossed with other breeds, the male proves to be homozygous, the female, heterozygous in barring. For the male Rock crossed with a non-barred breed produces only barred offspring in both sexes, but the female Rock crossed with the same non-barred breed produces offspring approximately half of which are barred, the other half being non- barred. Further, the barred individuals in this cross are invariably males, the non-barred ones being females. Accordingly, the distribution of barring and non-barring in the cross is sex- limited. The barred offspring produced by a cross between barred Plymouth Rocks and a non- barred breed, whether those offspring are males or females, prove to be heterozygous in barring, as we should expect, the barring factor having been received only from one parent, the barred one. Further, the non-barred offspring pro- duced by a barred Rock female crossed with a non-barred breed, do not transmit barring, hence they are pure recessives as regards bar- 171 HEREDITY ring. Hence, also, we are forced to conclude, as already suggested, the female of the pure barred Rock breed is heterozygous as re- gards barring, and transmits the character only to her male offspring, her female offspring (if the father is non-barred) neither being themselves barred nor being able to transmit barring. A pure Plymouth Bock race breeds true to barring merely because all its males are pure, for the females are not pure. This is shown by the following experiment. If a heterozygous barred male, produced by a cross between a Rock and a non-barred breed, is crossed with barred females, either those of a pure Rock race or those produced by a cross, the result is the same. The male offspring are all barred ; the females, half of them barred, half non- barred. This result shows that all barred females alike are heterozygous in barring. Sex-limited inheritance such as this finds at the present time its most probable explanation in the existence in the egg of an extra or plus element never found in the sperm, this element pairing with the sex-limited character in the 172 HEBEDITY AND SEX reduction division. Thus, in the barred Rock, calling barring B, the male of pure race is plainly B B and every sperm is B. But the female clearly contains only one B and can- FEMALE MALE (eg) FIG. 52. — Diagram of sex-limited inheritance when the female is a heterozygote, as in barred fowls. X, female sex deter- miner; B, barring. not be made to contain two. Perhaps a second B is kept out by some structural element, X, the distinctive structural element of the female individual. Then the eggs will be of two sorts: B and X. Since the sperms are all B, the first type of egg when fertilized will con- tain B B, a homozygous barred individual and 173 HEREDITY a male, since it lacks X; the second type will contain B X, a bird heterozygous in barring, and a female, since it contains X. This agrees with the experimental result (see Fig. 52). A heterozygous barred male will form two kinds of sperm, only one of which will contain B. If such a male be mated with a barred female, four sorts of zygotes should result, as follows : Gametes of heterozygous barred male = B and — , Gametes of barred female = B and X, Zygotes = B B (homozygous barred male) ; B — (heterozygous barred male), B'X (barred female), and —•X (non-barred female). The observed result of this cross accords fully with the foregoing expectation. The sex-limited inheritance of barring in fowls may be explained, as we have just seen, on the assumption that the female is the hetero- zygous sex. The same is true of sex-limited inheritance in canary-birds and in the moth, Abraxas, according to Bateson and Doncaster. But these relations are exactly reversed in the pomace-fly, Drosophila ampelophila, according to Morgan. 174 HEREDITY AND SEX In Drosophila the female is apparently homozygous as regards some cell-structure, X, which in the male is never represented more than once. Accordingly the formula of FEMALE MALE FIG. 53. — Diagram of sex-limited inheritance when the female is a homozygote, as in the red-eyed Drosophila. X, sex- determiner; R, red-eyes. the female is in such cases XX; that of the male, X — . Now the sex-limited characters in Drosophila seem to be bound up with the X structure, not repelled by it, as is barring in fowls. Accordingly, a sex-limited character may be represented twice in the female Droso- phila, but only once in the male; or in other 175 HEREDITY words, the female may be homozygous as re- gards a sex-limited character, but the male can only be heterozygous (see Fig. 53). Drosophila normally has red eyes, but the redness of the eye is a distinct unit-character, sex-limited in heredity. Further males are regularly heterozygous in this character, while females are homozygous. For Morgan has ob- tained a race in which the eyes are white, owing to the loss of the red character; and reciprocal crosses of this race with ordinary red-eyed animals yield different results. The red-eyed female crossed with a white-eyed male produces only red-eyed offspring, but the red- eyed male crossed with a white-eyed female produces offspring only half of which are red- eyed, viz. the females, whereas the males are white-eyed. These different results in the two cases ap- parently come about as follows: First case. Gametes of red-eyed female = X-R and X-R, Gametes of white-eyed male = X and — , Zygotes= X'X-R (red-eyed female), and = — -X-R (red-eyed male). 176 HEREDITY AND SEX Second case. Gametes of white-eyed female = X and X, Gametes of red-eyed male = X-R and — , Zygotes == X'X-R (red-eyed female), and — -X (white-eyed male). A short condition of the wings in Drosophila, which renders the animal incapable of flight, is likewise sex-limited in heredity, as has been shown by Morgan. By crossing two races of Drosophila, each of which possessed a different sex-limited character, Morgan has been able to combine the two characters in a single race. Thus was obtained a race both white-eyed and short-winged. The synthesis cannot be made originally in a male individual, but only in a female. For only in the female can the two characters be brought together, each associated with a different X, since in the male only one X is present. Although each sex-limited char- acter seems to be attached to or bound up with an X structure, it evidently has a material basis distinct from X. Otherwise it would not be possible for the character to leave one X and attach itself to the other, as apparently takes place in the female when the combination of 177 HEREDITY two sex-limited characters in the same gamete is secured through a cross. The combination is apparently secured in this way: Gametes uniting, X-R and X— L, Zygote formed, X-R'X-L, Its gametes, X-R and X-L, or X-R-L and X. One of the uniting gametes, X-R, is formed by the red-eyed, short- winged parent ; the other, X-L, is formed by the long-winged, white- eyed parent. The zygote resulting is a red- eyed individual, since it contains R ; it is long- winged, since it contains L ; it is a female, since it contains two Xs. Now, its gametes are of four sorts, as indicated. The first two sorts result from simple separation of the two Xs, each with its associated character, R in one case, L in the other. But the third sort could result only from the attachment of R and L to the same X, leaving the other X without either R or L as the fourth kind of gamete. This kind, which transmits neither red eyes nor long wings, would represent the new gametic combination,— white-eyed and with short wings. The experimental evidence that gametes of 178 HEREDITY AND SEX these four sorts are formed by females of the origin described is as follows : — When such a female is mated with a long- winged, white-eyed male, there are obtained female offspring, all of which are long- winged, but half of them are red-eyed, half white-eyed. The male offspring, however, are of four sorts, viz. red short, white long, red long, and white short. This result harmonizes with the hypothesis advanced. For if the gametes of the female are X-R, X-L, X-R-L, and X, and those of the male are X-L and — , then the following combinations should result : X-L' X-R, red long female, X-L' X-L, white long female X-L' X-R-L, red long female, X-L' X , white long female, • X-R, red short male, • X-L, white long male, • X-R-L, red long male, • X , white short male. This expected result accords with that ac- tually obtained by Morgan. Color-blindness in man is a sex-limited char- acter, the inheritance of which resembles that 179 HEBEDITY of white eyes or short wings in Drosophila, rather than of barring in poultry. Color-blindness is much commoner in men than in women. A color-blind man, however, does not transmit color-blindness to his sons, but only to his daughters, the daughters, how- ever, are themselves normal provided the mother was; yet they transmit color-blindness to half their sons. A color-blind daughter could be produced, apparently, only by the marriage of a color-blind man with a woman who transmitted color-blindness, since the daughter to be color-blind must have received the character from both parents, whereas the color-blind son receives the character only from his mother. Color-blindness is apparently due to a defect in the germ-cell, — absence of something nor- mally associated there with an X-structure, which is represented twice in woman, once in man. Color-blindness follows, therefore, in transmission the scheme shown in Fig 53. If, as has been suggested, the determination of sex in general depends upon the inheritance of a Mendelian factor differentiating the sexes, 180 HEREDITY AXD SEX it is highly improbable that the breeder will ever be able to control sex. Male and female zygotes should forever continue to be produced in approximate equality, and consistent inequal- ity of male and female births could result only from greater mortality on the part of one sort of zygote than of the other. Only in partheno- genesis can man at will control sex, and until he can produce artificial parthenogenesis in the higher animals, he can scarcely hope to con- trol sex in such animals. Negative as are the results of our study of sex- control, they are perhaps not wholly without practical value. It is something to know our limitations. We may thus save time from useless attempts at controlling what is uncon- trollable and devote it to more profitable em- ployments. BIBLIOGRAPHY BATESON, W. 1909. (See Bibliography to Chapter IV.) CASTLE, W. E. 1909. "A Mendelian View of Sex-heredity." Science, N. S., vol. 29, pp. 395-400. CUENOT, L. 1900. "Sur la determination du sexe chez les animaux." Bull. Sci. de la France et de la Belgique. 13 181 MORGAN, T. H. 1909. "A Biological and Cytological Study of Sex Deter- mination in Phylloxerans and Aphids." Journal of Experimental Zoology, 7, pp. 239-352. 1910. "Sex-limited Inheritance in Drosophila." Science, N. S., 32, pp. 120-122. 1911. "The Application of the Conception of Pure Lines to Sex-limited Inheritance and to Sexual Dimorphism." The American Naturalist, 45, pp. 65-78. Russo, A. 1909. "Studien liber die Bestimmung des weiblichen Geschlectes." G. Fischer, Jena. WILSON, E. B. 1909. "Recent Researches on the Determination and Heredity of Sex." Science, N. S., 29, pp. 53-70. 1910. "The Chromosomes in Relation to the Determina- tion of Sex." Science Progress, 5, pp. 570-592. For references to the earlier literature see CU^NOT and BATESON. INDEX Abraxas, sex determination in, 174. Atavism, 62. von Baehr, 165. Basile, 159. Bateson, 37, 110, 174. Baur, 61. Bos, 147. Buttercup, 107. Cattle, polled, 39, 102. Ciona, 146. Color blindness, 179. Correns, 34. Coutagne, 99. Cross-breeding, effects of, on vigor, 149. Cuenot, 158. Daphnia, 116, 164. Darwin, 7, 62, 106. Davenport, 100. Doncaster, 174. Drosophila, 146. sex limited inheritance in, 174. East, 130, 138. Egg, fertilization of, 11. of Nereis, 12. of mouse, 13. of sea-urchin, 18. Factors, inhibiting, 55. multiple, 62, 131. Farabee, 39. Fern, 20. prothallus of, 22. spores of, 22. Fingers, inheritance of short, 39. Fixation of a reversionary char- acter, 68. Fowls, Andalusian, 55. crosses of, 53. sex limited inheritance in, 170. Gamete, definition of, 25. Guinea-pig, angora, 38, 42, 47. black crossed with white, 34. new variety of, 84. polydactylous, 100, 121. reversion in, 63. rough crossed with smooth, 38, 41, 47, 98. Hare, Belgian, 30. 183 INDEX Heape, 30. Rats, 91. Heredity, collateral, 6. inbreeding in, 147. definition of, 6. selection in, 123. Hyalodaphnia, 117. Reduction, in fern, 22. Reversion, 62. Jennings, 111. Riddle, 87. Johannsen, 45, 106. Riley, 158. Rodents, coloration of, 72. Little, 59. Rotifers, sex determination in, 164. Maize, ear-length in, 138. Russo, 159. Maturation of egg, 15, 20. of sperm-cells, 17. Schenck, 159. Mendel, 7, 34. Self-fertilization, 145. Mendelian ratios, 35, 45, 50, 59. Sheep, horns in, 102. Mice, pale-colored, 81. Silk moths, 99. pink-eyed, 79. Snapdragon, golden variety of, spotted, 83. 61. yellow, 57. Spermatogenesis, 17. Morgan, 91, 165, 174. of wasp, 24. Mus alexandrinus, 91. Squash bug, sex determination Mus norvegicus, 94. in, 168. Mus rattus, 91, 94. Mutilations, inheritance of, 29. Tornier, 102. Transplantation of egg, 30. Nilsson-Ehle, 131. of ovary, 31. Treat, 158. Parthenogenesis, artificial, 18. sex determination in, 162. Unit-characters, 38. Pigeons, reversion in, 62. Phenotype, 45. De Vries, 34, 89, 106. Phillips, 31. Prepotency, 101. Weismann, 29, 148. Punnett, 159. Wilson, 168. Woltereck, 117. Rabbits, size inheritance in, 129. Zygote, definition of, 25. 184 (i) TWENTIETH CENTURY TEXT-BOOKS. TEXT-BOOKS OF ZOOLOGY. By DAVID STARR JORDAN, President of Leland Stan- ford Jr. University; VERNON LYMAN KELLOGG, Professor of Entomology; HAROLD HEATH, Assistant Professor of Invertebrate Zoology. Evolution and Animal Life. This is a popular discussion of the facts, processes, laws, and theories relating to the life and evolution of animals. The reader of it will have a very clear idea of the all-important theory of evolution as it has been developed and as it is held to-day by scientists. 8vo. Cloth, with about 300 illustrations, $2.50 net ; postage 20 cents additional. Animal Studies. A compact but complete treatment of elementary zoology, especially prepared for institutions of learning that prefer to find in a single book an ecological as well as morphological survey of the animal world. I2mo. Cloth, $1.25 net. Animal Life. An elementary account of animal ecology — that is, of the relations of animals to their surroundings. It treats of animals from the stand- point of the observer, and shows why the present conditions and habits of animal life are as we find them. I2mo. Cloth, $1.20 net. Animal Forms. This book deals in an elementary way with animal morphology. It describes the structure and life processes of animals, from the lowest creations to the highest and most complex. I2mo. Cloth, $1.10 net. Animals. This consists of "Animal Life" and "Animal Forms" bound in one volume. I2mo. Cloth, $1.80. Animal Structures. A laboratory guide in the teaching of elementary zoology. I2mo. Cloth, 50 cents net. D. APPLETON AND COMPANY, NEW YORK. BOSTON. CHICAGO. LONDON. TWENTIETH CENTURY TEXT-BOOKS. TEXT-BOOKS OF ZOOLOGY. By DAVID STARR JORDAN, President of Leland Stan- ford Jr. University; VERNON LYMAN KELLOGG, Professor of Entomology; HAROLD HEATH, Assistant Professor of Invertebrate Zoology. Evolution and Animal Life. This is a popular discussion of the facts, processes, laws, and theories relating to the life and evolution of animals. The reader of it will have a very clear idea of the all-important theory of evolution as it has been developed and as it is held to-day by scientists. 8vo. Cloth, with about 300 illustrations, $2.50 net ; postage 20 cents additional. Animal Studies. A compact but complete treatment of elementary zoology, especially prepared for institutions of learning that prefer to find in a single book an ecological as well as morphological survey of the animal world. I2mo. Cloth, $1.25 net. Animal Life. An elementary account of animal ecology — that is, of the relations of animals to their surroundings. It treats of animals from the stand- point of the observer, and shows why the present conditions and habits of animal life are as we find them. I2mo. Cloth, $1.20 net. Animal Forms. This book deals in an elementary way with animal morphology. It describes the structure and life processes of animals, from the lowest creations to the highest and most complex, ramo. Cloth, $1.10 net. Animals. This consists of " Animal Life " and " Animal Forms " bound in one volume. I2mo. Cloth, $1.80. Animal Structures. A laboratory guide in the teaching of elementary zoology. I2mo. Cloth, 50 cents net. D. APPLETON AND COMPANY, NEW YORK. BOSTON. CHICAGO. LONDON. SCIENCE, RELIGION, EDUCATION. Adolescence : Its Psychology and Its Relations to Physiology, Anthropology, Sociology, Sex, Crime, Religion, and Education. By G. STANLEY HALL, Ph.D., LL.D. Two vols., royal 8vo, gilt top. Cloth, $7.50 net. This work is the result of many years of study and teaching. It is the first attempt in any language to bring together all the best that has been ascertained about the critical period of life which begins with puberty in the early teens and ends with maturity in the middle twenties, and it is made by the one man whose experience and ability pre-emi- nently qualify him for such a task. The work includes a summary of the author's conclusions after twenty-five years of teaching and study upon some of the most important themes in ' Philosophy, Psychology, Religion, and Education. The nature of the adolescent period is the best guide to education from the upper grades of the grammar school through the high school and college. Throughout, the statement of scientific facts is followed systematically by a consideration of their application to education, pe- nology, and other phases of life. Juvenile diseases and crime have each special chapters. The changes of each sense during this period are taken up. The study of normal psychic life is introduced by a chapter describing both typical and excep- tional adolescents, drawn from biography, literature, lives of the saints, and other sources. The practical applications of some of the conclusions of the scientific part are found in separate chapters on the education of girls, coeduca- tion and its relations to marriage, fecundity, and family life, as seen by statistics in American colleges, with a sketch of an ideal education for girls. Another chapter treats with some detail and criticism the various kinds and types of organization for adolescents from plays and games to the Y. M. C. A., Epworth League, and other associations devised for the young. The problem of the High School, its chief topics and methods, is considered from the standpoint of adolescence, and some very important modifications are urged. It closes with the general consideration of the relations of a higher to a lower civilization from this standpoint D. APPLETON AND COMPANY, NEW YORK.