VOL. XLVIII, NO. 565 L The American Naturalist MSS. intended for publication and books, etc., intended for review should be sent to the E Editor of THE on NATURALIST, Garrison-on-Hudson, New York, articles cont summaries of research work bearing on the problems ns of or organic Siete eo especially welcome, and will be given preference One hundrea reprints of contributions are supplied to authors free of charge. Further reprints will ed Hp sa at cost Subscriptions and advertisem aati should be = to the publishers. The erten S dollars a year. Ai hi ostage is ps cents and ye Ci 5 — > THE SCIENCE PRESS — | _ NEW YORK : Sub-Station 84 Entered i ‘ , April 2, 1908, t the Post Offi ter, Pa., under the Act ot = Soe = “FOR SALE a | TENTH EDITION. ARCTIC, ICELAND and GREENLAND ii oe te li ae — THE Se Ore, THE AMERICAN NATURALIST TAE AMERICAN NATURALIST A MONTHLY JOURNAL DEVOTED TO THE ADVANCEMENT OF THE BIOLOGICAL SCIENCES WITH SPECIAL REFERENCE TO THE FACTORS OF EVOLUTION VOLUME XLVIII - NEW YORK THE SCIENCE PRESS 1914 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST Vout. XLVIII January, 1914 No. 565 A GENETIC ANALYSIS OF THE CHANGES PRO- DUCED BY SELECTION IN EXPERIMENTS WITH TOBACCO! PROFESSOR E. M. EAST axb H. K. HAYES BUSSEY [INSTITUTION OF HARVARD UNIVERSITY THE PROBLEM In 1903 Johannsen announced that continued selection of the extreme values of certain quantitative characters in successive self-fertilized generations of a number of strains of beans had produced no changes in the. mean values of the characters. He concluded that these par- ticular strains were homozygous for the gametic factors whose interaction resulted in the characters investigated, that these homozygous characters may be properly de- scribed by one or more gametic factors nonvariable in transmissible qualities and properties, and that the varia- tions observed in the characters of any single fraternity were due entirely to the action of environmental condi- tions during ontogeny and were not inherited. Funda- mentally, these conclusions were a recognition of the gen- eral value of Mendelian description for all forms of in- heritance through sexual reproduction, combined with an 1 These investigations were conducted with funds furnished by the Con- necticut Agricultural Experiment Station from their Adams’ appropria- tions, by the Bureau of Plant Industry of the United States Department of Agriculture, and by the Bussey Institution of Harvard University, and the writers desire to take this opportunity of expressing their sincere appre- ciation of this hearty cooperation which made the work possible. 5 6 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII admission of disbelief in the inheritance of ordinary adaptive changes. The latter conception was Weismann- ian in that all inherited variations were held to be changes in the germ cells. It was not necessary to suppose it im- possible for the environment to produce such changes and therefore to have been of no value during the course of evolution, but merely to suppose that during the compara- tively short period of experimental investigations no gam- etic variations have occurred traceable to such a cause. For his first conclusion to be justified, it was assumed that the changes which every biologist knows do follow the continuous selection of extremes under certain conditions are to be interpreted entirely by the segregation and re- combination of hypothetical gametie factors which are constant in their reactions under identical conditions. Numerous investigators working on ‘‘pure lines’’ with different material corroborated Johannsen’s conclusions, and, as it was seen to be possible to interpret in the same manner changes made by selection in experiments where self-fertilized lines were not used, such as those of the Vilmorins and others on sugar beets and those of the Illinois Agricultural Experiment Station on maize, many biologists accepted them and considered them a great ad- vance over former conceptions of the mechanism of heredity. On the other hand, there were those who main- tained a skeptical attitude, the chief criticism directed against the conception being that all progress due to selection must have a limit, which in many of these ex- periments had already been reached, and that even if re- sults were being obtained action might be too slow to be detected. THE MATERIAL These criticisms were reasonable when applied to cer- tain specific cases, and in 1908 the experiments reported in this paper were designed with the hope of testing their validity, using the species ordinarily grown for commer- cial tobacco, Nicotiana tabacum, as the material. This plant satisfies the conditions which are requisite for No. 565] CHANGES PRODUCED BY SELECTION 7 material used in pure line studies. It has characters that can be estimated readily and accurately and which are affected only slightly by external conditions. It is easily grown, is naturally self-fertilized, reproduces prolifically, and is known in many markedly different varieties. In fact, it is an ideal subject for work of this kind. The investigations were not patterned after the stand- ard type set by Johannsen wherein the constancy of suc- cessive generations of pure lines grown from selected extremes were tested, since even if it were possible to gather a quantity of data at all comparable to that col- lected by Johannsen (:09) and Jennings (:08) in their brilliant investigations, the criticisms mentioned above might still be made. The plan chosen was that of cross- ing two varieties of tobacco which differed in a character complex easily and precisely determined, and of selecting extremes from a number of families of the F, generation. If Johannsen’s views be incorrect, such continued selec- tion should affect each family in the same degree. If his conclusions be justified, selection should reach an end- point in different generations in different families, and there should be no relation between the number of genera- tions required to reach this end-point and the progress that is possible. There should be no need of a historical summary of the previous investigations that have been interpreted as cor- roborating or refuting Johannsen’s conclusions. Such summaries have been made in other papers. It should be mentioned, however, that the classical researches of Pearl (:11) on the inheritance of fecundity in the domestic fowl have been so planned and executed that certain of the criticisms directed against Johannsen mentioned above are not justified, yet Pearl finds himself thoroughly in accord with the Danish physiologist’s position. Several hundred varieties of Nicotiana tabacum exist which differ from each other by definite botanical char- acters, yet only two general characters suitable for our purpose were found. We desired to confine our observa- tions to quantitative characters that were influenced but 8 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII little by environment, and number of leaves and size of corolla were the only ones that satisfied this requirement. Such character differences as height of plant and size of leaf, while undoubtedly transmissible, are influenced so strongly in their development by nutrition that work with them is exceedingly difficult. For example, if a certain variety of Nicotiana tabacum is grown under the best of field conditions, the longest leaves are about 28 inches and the total height about 6 feet, but a portion of the same seed fraternity may be grown to maturity in 4-inch pots without reaching a height of over 16 inches or having leaves longer than 4 inches. On the other hand, several experiments conducted in the same manner have shown no difference between the frequency curves of variation in number of leaves or of size of corolla, whether starved in small pots or grown under optimum conditions. The character complex number of leaves was chosen for this investigation rather than the size of corolla because vari- eties that differ greatly in number of leaves are common. TABLE I FREQUENCY DISTRIBUTION OF NUMBER OF LEAVES PER PLANT WHEN ARVED IN SMALL POTS (Compare with frequency distribution under normal field conditions at Forest Hills, Massachusetts, in Tables VII and XI) No. of Leaves per Plant Plant No. 22 | 23 | 24 | 25 | 26 | 27 | 28 | 29 | 30 | 31 | 32 | 33 | 34 | 35 | 36 | 37 (6-1) SSO AS) SE Tih: (6-1)-1 1 SOG TI2 ph tee eres (6-2) bara 1 Sp o¢ (347161 14) 8S) Sai, (6-2)-2 RER E S OL bh Oi, op o AD e ee (56-1) solk Pe SiR OE TT 2a a (56-2) EPE a a Lie beh Ol a EE Previous Work or THE ‘‘Havana’’ X ‘‘SumatrRa’’ Cross Several crosses have been made between varieties of tobacco that had a mean difference of seven or eight leaves, but the majority of the data reported here were collected from the descendants of a cross made by Shamel between the types known in Connecticut as ‘*Havana’’ and ‘‘Sumatra.’’ The ‘‘Havana’’ parent was No. 565] CHANGES PRODUCED BY SELECTION 9 from a variety that had been grown for a number of years at Granby, Connecticut. It averages about 20 leaves per plant although ranging from 16 to 25 leaves. The aver- age height is about 1.4 m. and the average leaf area about 7 sq. dm. The ‘‘Sumatra’’ parent was a type specimen of a variety that had been introduced into Connecticut to be grown under cloth shade. It averages between 26 and 27 leaves per plant with a range of from 21 to 32 leaves. The average height is nearly 2.0 m., but the average leaf area is only about 3 sq. dm. According to Shamel, the first hybrid generation of this cross developed somewhat more vigorously than the parent types and was uniform in its habit of growth. The second generation, he thought, was hardly more vari- able than the first. Several F, families, the progeny of inbred F, individuals, were grown in 1906 and proved to be a variable lot. One of these plants produced 26 small, round-pointed leaves with short internodes between them. This plant was thought by Mr. E. Halladay, upon whose farm the experiment was conducted, and Mr. J. B. Stewart, of the U. S. Department of Agriculture, to be worth sav- ing from its promise of producing a desirable commercial type. In 1907 the Department of Agriculture made an agree- ment with Mr. Halladay to grow two acres of tobacco for experimental purposes, and on his own initiative Mr. Halladay grew a number of plants from inbred seed of the one that bore 26 leaves. This selection, numbered 2 h-29 in accordance with the department nomenclature, was comparatively uniform in appearance and several plants were selfed. In Mr. Halladay’s absence, how- ` ever, all of the plants were ‘‘topped,’’ except one that happened to be rather late. This plant was selfed. It had 26 medium-sized, round leaves and grew to about the same height as the Connecticut Havana. In view of Mr. Halladay’s high opinion of the type, the seed of this plant and the remaining seed of its parent were planted in 1908. The plants of this generation pre- sented a uniform appearance and promised a high grade 10 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII of wrapper tobacco, but the crop when cured lacked uni- formity. Some leaves of exceptionally high quality were produced, but the crop in general lacked that characteris- tic known as ‘‘grain’’ and had too large a proportion of heavy leaves—the so-called ‘‘tops.”’ From this 1908 generation 100 seed plants were selfed, their leaves harvested, cured and fermented separately, and data on quality recorded. The type was also grown commercially on a large scale. The commercial results, however, have been reported in another paper. Weare to consider only the results of the selection experiment that began in 1908, through the cooperation between the U. S. Department of Agriculture and the Connecticut Agricul- tural Experiment Station, a joining of forces that in 1909 included the Bussey Institution of Harvard University. Shamel ( :07) considered the strain produced by this cross to be the result of a mutation. From a study of the data from the previous work on the cross it seemed to the writers that a different interpretation of the results might be made. While it was not impossible that the many- leaved type that had been isolated was the result of a mutation, it appeared much more probable that it had arisen through a recombination of Mendelian factors. The type had the habit of growth and size of leaf of the pure ‘‘Havana’’ variety and the number of leaves of the ‘‘Sumatra’’ variety, a combination that might reason- ably be expected to be the result of the Mendelian law. RESULTS ON THE RECIPROCAL Cross, ‘‘SumMaTra’’ X “HAVANA” To test the hypothesis that the new tobacco was the result of such recombination and could be reproduced whenever desired, the reciprocal of the original cross was made in 1910. The female parent, ‘‘Sumatra,’’ was the direct descendant of a sister of the plant used as the male parent of the original cross by Shamel in 1903 through seven generations of selfed plants. The male parent, ‘‘Havana,’’ was from the commercial field of the Windsor Tobacco Growers’ Corporation at Bloomfield, No. 565] CHANGES PRODUCED BY SELECTION 11 Connecticut. It was a descendant in a collateral line of the plant used by Shamel in 1903 as the female parent in his cross. Table II, giving the frequency distribution for the num- ber of leaves of the two parents and the first and the second hybrid generations, is a complete justification of our prediction as to how the hybrid type produced by Shamel originated. The ‘‘Sumatra’’ and the F, genera- tion were grown at New Haven, Connecticut, in 1911, the ‘‘Havana’’ was grown at Bloomfield, Connecticut, in 1911 from commercial seed of the same variety as the plant used for the male parent, while the F, generation was grown at New Haven, Connecticut, in 1912. The F, gen- eration, producing an average of 23.3+.14 leaves per plant, is intermediate in leaf number, since the ‘‘ Havana’’ variety shows an average leaf number per plant of 19.8 + .08 and the ‘‘Sumatra’’ variety 26.5+.11. The varia- tion as determined by the coefficient of variability is some- what less for the F, than for either parent. The value for the ‘‘Sumatra’’ variety is 6.64 per cent. +.28 per cent., for the ‘‘Havana’’ variety 6.98 per cent. + .27 per cent. and for the F, generation 6.24 per cent. +.41 per cent. Taking into consideration the probable error in each case, one may say that the variability of the three populations is almost the same. The variability of the F, generation, however, is greatly increased. This is shown by the high coefficient of vari- ability, 10.29 -+ .23 per cent., although a glance at the fre- quency distribution with its range of from 18 to 31 leaves brings home the point without recourse to biometrical calculation. The appearance of the plants in the field corroborated the data of Table II in other characters. The F, genera- tion was intermediate in the various leaf characters, such as shape, size and texture, that distinguish ‘‘Sumatra”’ from ‘‘Havana’’ tobacco, and in these characters it seemed as uniform as either of the parental varieties. On the other hand, the F, generation was extremely variable. Some plants could not be distinguished from the pure ‘‘Suma- 12 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vor. XLVIII tra,’’ others resembled ‘‘ Havana,’’ although of course the majority were intermediate in various degrees. Several plants combined the leaf size and habit of growth of the ‘‘Havana’’ parent with the leaf number of the ‘‘Suma- tra’’ parent. In other words, plants were produced in the F, generation by the recombination of Mendelian fac- tors that exactly repeated the type which Shamel had ob- tained in the F, generation of the reciprocal cross made in 1903 and which he thought was due to a mutation. This fulfilled adequately the prediction made by us in 1908. RESULTS or SELECTING ror High NUMBER anp Low Num- BER OF LEAVES IN THE ‘‘Havana’’ ‘‘SumatTRA’’ Cross In describing the reproduction of Shamel’s hybrid with numerous large leaves by a reciprocal cross, there has been a chronological inversion. This was done simply to show that the original hybrid known commercially as “The Halladay’’ was actually a recombination of Men- delian factors in which the ‘‘Havana’’ and the ‘‘Suma- tra’’ varieties differed. We will now describe the effects of selection on the original ‘‘ Halladay hybrid.”’ It will be recalled that the selection experiment which is the principal subject of this paper began with the self- ing of 100 seed plants of Shamel’s Halladay hybrid in 1908. These plants were the F, and F, generations of the cross ‘‘Havana’’ X ‘‘Sumatra.’’ Plants numbered from 1 to 49 were the F, generation; those numbered from 50 to 100 were the F, generation. They were apparently breeding true for the short habit of growth and large- sized leaf of the ‘‘Havana’’ parent and the goodly num- ber of leaves of the ‘‘Sumatra’’ parent. The casual ob- server either would have said with Shamel that here was a mutation breeding as true as any tobacco variety, or that a fixed hybrid, a hybrid homozygous in all of its gametic factors, had been produced. Accurate data taken on the progeny of those of the F, and F, seed plants which it was possible for us to grow in our limited space, No. 565] CHANGES PRODUCED BY SELECTION 13 however, show that such judgments would have been superficial. The general type of the plant did appear to be fixed, but the frequency distribution for number of leaves of the F, and F, populations were not the same. Strictly speaking, they were not fixed. What would be the result of selecting (and selfing) extremes from these different families for a number of years? A tentative answer to this question is to be obtained by examining the remainder of our tables. The tables are arranged roughly in the order of the effect that selection has had in changing the mean of the various families that were the starting points of this part of the experiment. The selections were grown near Bloom- field, Connecticut, on the light sandy loam of that region, soil typical of that which produces the famous Connecti- cut River Valley wrapper tobacco. Duplicate experi- ments with several of the original families were made at New Haven, Connecticut, however, on an impoverished soil not fitted to grow a good quality of tobacco even after supplying large quantities of tobacco fertilizer, and in the condition used not fitted to grow good crops of any kind. Two families were also grown in triplicate, the third selections being planted at Forest Hills, Massachu- setts, on a very fine type of rich garden land which brought out maximum luxuriance of growth, but which did not produce good tobacco quality. These experiments were not true repetitions of the experiments at Bloomfield, Connecticut, since aliquot portions of the seed from the selfed plant grown there were not sent to the other places to be grown. But they were duplicates in that each family came from the same F, or F, mother plant, although, beginning with the F, or F, population, differ- ent selfed seed plants furnished the starting point of selec- tions carried on independently. In this way there were afforded a greater number of chances to see what selec- tion could do. Table III shows the results obtained from family No. 77. This family arose from an F, plant having 23 leaves, one below the modal leaf number if we may judge from [Vou. XLVIII THE AMERICAN NATURALIST 14 sooovaoy, (TIVW) Vulvwoag ANV (TIVNA) VNVAVH NIIMLIg SSONQ NI INVIG Ud SHAVA'T JO YAAWON AO NOILAJINLSIA AONTNOAAT Il @TaviL 12’ * 8F'L | 90°+ £61 | 80° + 8S [08z) T | T| F |£ | st| 6c SF] v9] OS | FFI See |T "Tl" "1 Q8(ZI6T | 8H] = T-(Z—-22) 02° * Z6'9 | GO" F8'T | 0° 9°9G |T8Z| T| T|? | ZL| Z| 1F| 6F| 89| SE} Fe) OT! Z LATE Se" 1 Oe TOT 1 Ae I-(Z-L2) OEF ZEG | LOF SES | Il = OFS PEZ) T|o/¢ |9 | SL} 22] OF | 68} SE} FE} ST) IL) F ; ri "|tt t] 26 1OT6L | 4A (€L) EEF 8E'9j LO F SST | Il FEISS | we ree (ate emma patie cms Oe Gan yt ak ee a ole ek CARR sige CEOE Cee GL | tae LL LZ = L76 | 90° = $0°Z | 80° 6'13 (993); Zz |z |6 | 12| 02| 28| ZF| €S| 6F| OZ|¢ |T “°°! OZ |OT6T | 4a (1-22) 8T°'+09'9 | F0°= TFT | 90°F SIs 908} ° eS TS Peet Sot 261881 Oe re oe **) OS |TIGT | 84 I-(1-L4) 96° LES | 0° = FST | LO°F FST [OES * : T IT |g |F 19 | Te} se] 29] 9F| OT] © | T | Of\zr6T | SH) Z-1-(1-44) e ze te) jae |se | az | om |s [ve |se |ze| ra| oz [or or] ar or [or ni Bal | = = A0 ‘a's vy AE SE 2 S ON que[g sad seave'y jo Iəquny JR S 3 + ‘NNOO ‘CTAIANWNOOTgQ LV NMOUDY ‘LL ATIWV NI SUAVA'T JO WAAWAN AO NOILNSINISIA AONTAOTYT OI WIdVib EZ = OZOTT | 90°= 19°C | 80°+ F'F2| O8F |S 12 | SL} SS| 6F| 6S) ILI: 99 SF] 88i IS 6 jl POC “tq ‘vuvaey X seung lesen | Oe ort) stl egec| cf fl ee ee ee ee i te e r. tease ak Suvas X Sa 82'=F9'9 |80=9LT|IT'=S97| SZI |T |+ | ST|9T| 12) 9s] zjete pop P ee ees veageumg 2z°=86°9 looser sorger) ost cll ell ct te lo lorlecleplepiee| epee Mets Fs : ` + + ouea sH Ts | og | 6z | s3 | 23 | 9% | sz | #3 | eg | zz | 13 | OZ | GE | St} LT AD ‘a's y IL verser FN quvig 13d seavay jo soquinyy 15 CHANGES PRODUCED BY SELECTION No. 565] ‘Buyyqnop Jeo] pomoys syuerd zo requmu y p *{NOTPOUNOD PPPYUWOO Je UMOIH g st +8z9 | 90 +691T | LO*69Z | S22 | 1 | Z| tl 2e| $9169] 6G seoir I 6z Z161 Ti 1-1-(2-92) Za’ * 889 90° = 08'T 80° = 19% OZ | T| 119 | 02] #2) S| 87i eio |Z 8% TIGI 3 1-(2-92) 12’ * 129 SOEST oF FP | 603 ||" | 1 |1 19 | OT| 22109 \#¢ | OF| OT! F lz O16 y (z-94) Pe = F9'9 80° = 09°T IU +1 | 68 ‘eeft lp letleries istie | welt! gz 6061 94 Zo" = L0°9 el = EST etraz | Te reige die P je jo1olr| ee OT6I i (1-92) FL + 82'S £0° = 92'T GO’ ¥ 6'&% org T 16 |8I 99 SOT CL 96) 911 1 e TI6I Ki T-(1-92) i ze | 1e | og | 6z | sz | 23 | 9% | s | Fe | sz | zz | IZ | og mozeg Jo sy ‘as Vv 1810, | poet Lag TOD | o ‘ON qud 19d savory Jo Iəquny | 1əquny 5 ‘NNOO ‘CTIIAWOOTE LY NMOUD a L XIUNV,T NI SAVI'I JO YAAWON AO NOILAJINLSIA AONTAdAUT AI ATSAViL ¥0' * IL'Z | OL = 62'S | FI = 2°63 [81] 3| 0 | 2 | Z]E€|OT|IT|ZI|OE|Eege| IT] Tt |T “T1711 OB 1ST6L | S| ATD 62’ ZL | 80° Z6'T | TT = 9'°9Z PRT] || g |9 | ST] SS TS fe OS Sti 2 1B ci iol es iter | tax I-(Z-LL) 18" * $2'6 | 80°= G8" | IL" FEZ |102|’ 1/%2{|% {ie |Z | OL} St| ez] ve} 2e| ge] te] ort lt "| 2g |O16T | 4a (-LL) €°= 8E°9 | L0°* SEL | Il FSS IS8 |) pei Teee eao 18 101 os e ET) Ot EZ |6061 | 9q ell 12° = L8°8| 90° 061 | 60°F 2°20 (FIZ) | cyt 10 10 e 1% «LZ ISL) 22] 1S] | cel stis OZ |OI6T 4a (I-22) ESF 19'S SO FEST Le LIZ Pee € |9 |Sz| orl IF] 211% ` `| Og |TT6T | 34 T-(1-44) GZ’ = LI'9 | S0'= 62'T | 80°= 6'02 (SET) IT |6 |8| FF] 6%| OT) 2) 1] TZ \ster |51| T-1-(1-42) e oe | oe | ve | ee) ae | we | ee) oz | g | oe [os lve) os [o| wa | os or orien Bad |g E 5 B “A°0 ‘a's v <3 se $ 3 ON a’ La | que} q 19d seavery Jo soquinN Fe sis [=] ot ‘NNOO ‘NUAV]T MIN LV NMOWÐ 16 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (VoL. XLVIII the F, generation of the reciprocal cross where the mode was at 24 to 25 leaves. The F, fraternity that it pro- duced was somewhat smaller than one would wish if he were to, be confident of the calculations made. The mode is 22 leaves and the mean nearly the same, 22.4 +.11 leaves. From among these plants, a minus variant having 20 leaves and a plus variant having 27 leaves were selected to produce the F, generation. The modes in this generation are 21 and 25 leaves, respectively, a difference of 4 leaves; and the means are 21.9+.08 and 24.9+ .11 leaves, respectively, a difference of 3 leaves. Progress in both directions continued when a 20-leaved plant was selected to carry on the minus strain, and a 30-leaved plant was selected to carry on the plus strain. The modal classes of the F, generation are 21 leaves in the minus selection and 26 leaves in the plus selection, while the means are 21.3 + .05 leaves and 26.6 + .07 leaves, respect- ively. In the F, generation the plus selection was lost, but the minus selection grown from a 20-leaved plant had the mode dropped to 18 leaves and the mean to 18.4 + .08 leaves. In order not to lose the plus selection entirely, however, more of the F, generation seed was grown in 1912. The mode is the same as in 1911, but the mean dropped slightly to 25.8 + .08 leaves. Here one notices what is very common throughout the experiment ; the extremes selected for mother plants were not members of the most extreme classes. This means simply that vigorous healthy specimens were always selected as the mother plants, and often the most extreme variants did not come up to the standard. It is hardly just to criticize this procedure, however, for with the best care that it was possible to give, the experiments with several families were terminated on account of non- germination of seed or for some similar reason, it being impossible, on account of the pressure of other work, to self many plants in each selection. Even where seed from several mother plants was collected, it did not in- sure the continuation of that selection. The necessary space and care involved in growing so many seedlings in 17 CHANGES PRODUCED BY SELECTION No. 565] “PIOFMOCT, YU UMOIH g oc" ELO |S "= ZOC|IL'+OO8| OOL | | 9] Z| St| 18/98) 0E6166 |F IT |T ZE ZI6I ta Z-I-(Z-61) 8z°* 10°9 | 80'= OL'T | II" 88%} 80T 1] 0/8 |£ | OT) 20)\ 28] 13| Ft1z 6% TI6T | 4a 1-(-61) 61° = PSS | GO'F OST | 20°F T'LZ| OGT "l'""1S |L | 28|OP| 4h) PE/ 9s 2 l 6% OI6L i (-61) LG * 189 | LOF 6L°T | OTF E92; LPI Z | Sl| S| PE] 6Z| S| 21/9 | FL TI’ 9% 606T g s61 SIT'= 82'S | S0'=F SE 90'F S'S) 66I "esiet 2 OTOP 29) 8h/ ST) Si Ti] we oter | *a (I-61) Zh + 628 | Il’ + 80° | STF 1'SZ) 06 outteesie**i@ T 78 1OLGT ZEIGE o 1 £t rt) e TIGI 4a T-(1-61) 6T'=P8'F| SOF ZET | SOF ELS) SET treg |F | 20/08] 12] 62S |8 hoe TIGI ‘x | iiA. eg | +e | ee | ze | te | og | 6z | sz | zz | 92 | sz | +z | ez | zz | ta | LI uamea jo “A‘O ‘a's ‘v PL soavoy jo, OOD | on ‘ON queg tod seave'y yo soquiny - | coquiny A uap "NNOO ‘NIAYVH MIN ZV Nowy 0% * IT'9 | 90° = SLT | 80°F S63) SIZ S12 | Fl SS] C9 ee re Ors Fit ss oe TIGI gi 2-(2-61) 92° FIL | 0° GOS | OTF 2°8Z| GAT | T| T | 1 | S| 2 | Sl) 8S| OF] 8Z| HST |S p po 62 OI6I i (Z-61) 12° = 18'9 | L0 6L°T | OTF 8°92 | LPT peer eS Sl eel eel eres Chie | Ft 9% 606T “fT 61 IZ’ = $9°9 | 90°F OL'T | 80°F 69% | LES "13 13 | GL| 92) €h| 89) T| 9S) 6T) Se || #3 OI6I i (1-61) SZ'= 97'4 | L0`= L8°T | 60°F 897| OGT I|T IT |F |4 91|9|90 oelet ejt] z TIGI 1 1-(1-61) 9g | ce | +e | se | ze | te | og | 6g | 83 | Lz | 9 | 9z | FZ | sz | zz | IZ |JUeABqJO AO a'S Y IPL soawoy jo; TAND | TOR ‘ON qux[g ted soave'y yo oquiny qoquinN A aÐ *NNOQ ‘TIMIANO LY NMOUH ‘6[ ATINV,[ NI SAAVTF'I JO SAAWAN dO NOILAIINISIQ AONATNOay,T A WTavVib e 18 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vor. XLVIII isolated seed pans filled with sterilized soil made it im- possible to start more than two sets of plants for each plus and each minus selection. Generally both sets grew perfectly, but occasionally both failed, and in that case it was usually too late in the season to start a third set even if it were available. The second part of Table III shows the results obtained on the poor soil of New Haven, Connecticut, with the same family. There was continuous progress in both direc- tions. The minus selections during the three generations show a constant reduction of mode, the figures being 23, 22 and 21; the plus selections show an even greater in- crease in mode, the figures being 25, 27 and 28. The same decrease and increase occur in the means until in the F, generation there is a difference of nearly 9 leaves, the cal- culated means being 20.9+.08 leaves and 29.7+.14 leaves, respectively. Figs. 1 and 2 show typical plants of the plus and minus strains of this family as developed by 3 years of selection. Fig. 3 illustrates an interesting change of phyllotaxy in some plants of (77—2)-1-1 as grown at New Haven in 1912. Passing to the data on Family No.76 (Table IV) there is the same evidence of the effectiveness of selection, ex- cluding the minus strain in 1910, of which only 31 plants were healthy. This effect is markedly less than with the other family. The mode of the minus selection remained at 24 leaves and the mean was reduced only from 24.1 + .11 leaves to 23.9 +.05 leaves,—hardly a significant figure. The mode of the plus selection crept up to 26-27 and the mean to 26.9 + .07 leaves, there being here one more generation than in the case of the minus strain. Table V gives the data on plus and minus selections of Family No. 19 at Bloomfield for two generations. The original family stock of the F, generation has the mode at 27 leaves and the mean at about 26 leaves. A 24-leaved plant of this generation became the parent of the minus strain, giving in the F, generation a population with the same mode and a slightly higher mean (26.9 + .08 leaves). Continuation of the strain through a 24-leaved plant gave No. 565] CHANGES PRODUCED BY SELECTION 19 an F, population with the mode one class lower and the mean at 25.8 + .09 leaves. Whether this slight reduction really means anything we are unable to say. it yields at all to selection, the progress is very slow. On the other hand, a con- siderable gain has been made in the plus selec- tions. The mode rose im- mediately to 29 leaves when the progeny of a 29- leaved plant were grown, and went up to 30 leaves the next generation, the modal condition being the same as the number of leaves of the parent plant. The means are 26.3+.-10 leaves, 28.7+.10 leaves and 29.2 + .08 leaves, the amount of progress being —as may be seen—2.4 leaves and 0.5 leaf in the two successive genera- tions. This result appar- ently indicates a slowing down of the effect of selec- tion. The continuation of the table gives the results ob- tained at New Haven on this same family. Here there are data from three generations, and these data modify the conclu- sions based on the results At least, if 1 X F HALLADAY HA- vANA Tosacco (77-2)-1-1, WHICH AV- PLANT IN 1909. Haven, 1912. obtained at Bloomfield. Both plus and minus strains nearly parallel the Bloomfield results for two generations, 20 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII the F, generation means being 28.3 + .11 leaves and 25.1 + .15 leaves, respectively, but in the F, generations they differ. Selecting minus extremes for the first two genera- È PLANT OF HALLADAY HAVANA Topacco (77-1)-1-1, WHICH AVERAGES 20.9 LEAVES PER PLANT. IT IS THE RESULT OF THREE YEARS OF SELECTION FOR Low Lear NUMBER IN FAMILY 77. New HAvEN, 1912. tions reduced the mean of that line from 26.3 + .10 leaves to 25.1 + .15 leaves, but the third selected generation (F,) had a higher mean than the original family (27.3 + .08 leaves). The parent plant of this F, generation produced No.565] CHANGES PRODUCED BY SELECTION 21 24 leaves, and as the strain indicated that it was hetero- zygous for a number of factors by showing a coefficient of variability of 8.29 + .42 per cent., it is possible that the selected parent plant may have belonged gametically to a higher class than was indicated somatically ; nevertheless, it can not be denied that three generations of selected minus extremes have produced no results. This conclu- sion is not valid for the plus strain. Starting with 26.3 + .10 as the mean number of leaves (F,), the succeeding gen- erations had means of 27.1+ .07 leaves, 28.3 + .11 leaves and 30.0 + .11 leaves. The differences are 0.8, 1-2 and 1.7 leaves, respectively. Progressive change has certainly fol- Fic. 3. CHANGE OF PHYLLOTAXY IN SOME PLANTS OF (77-2)-1-1 GROWN IN New HAVEN IN 1912. 22 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII lowed, and unless one considers that the results of 1912 are somewhat too high (probably a valid assumption), the change has increased instead of decreased. Naturally there must be a decreased momentum in change of mean time, but this decrease is not yet shown by the figures. Fig, PLANT OF HALLADAY HA- Fie. 5. PLANT oF HALLADAY Ha- VANA ToBacco (19-2)-1-2, WHICH Av- vyAana Tosacco (19-1)-1-1, WHICH Av- E E 0 LbBAvEs PER PLANT, IT ERAGES 27.3 LEAVES PER PLANT. THREE IS THE RESULT OF THREE Years OF Su- YEARS OF ŠELECTION FOR Low LEAF LECTION FOR HIGH LEAF NUMBER IN NUMBER HAVE PROVED UNSUCCESSFUL, FAMILY 19, WHICH IN 1909 AvmRracED New Haven, 1912. 26.3 LEAVES PER PLANT. New HAVEN, 1912. No.565] CHANGES PRODUCED BY SELECTION 23 Representative plants of the plus and minus strains of family 19 as obtained by three years of selection at New Haven are shown in Figs. 4 and 5. Family No. 5 (Table VI) shows a decrease in mode from 28 to 26 leaves, and a similar decrease in mean from 28.1 + .06 leaves to 26.6 + .09 leaves as a result of the first minus selection. A second minus selection, however, in- dicates either that the future progress is to be very slow or that the entire effect of selection was manifested in the first selected generation. With the three parts of Table VII we take up the re- sults on Family No. 6 at all three stations. The minus strain was carried on only two generations at Bloomfield, but with this exception there are data upon three genera- tions. At Bloomfield the two generations of selected minus extremes resulted in 0.6 leaf decrease in the mean, but at New Haven the results were negative, the means advancing from 25.8 + .06 leaves to 27.9+.12 leaves in three generations, while at Forest Hill the mean remained practically the same. Surely selection was unprofitable ere. The first year of selection from the other end of the curve, however, resulted in marked progress. The mean advanced nearly 5 leaves in each case. The original F, mean is 25.8 + .06 leaves, but the three F, means are 30.7 + .09, 29.6 + .08 and 30.8+.12 leaves. This is a remark- able concurrence of results. The means in the two suc- ceeding generations were about the same in the Bloomfield and New Haven experiments, but there was another defi- nite advance at Forest Hills. Such a result should not be unexpected. If the F, generation were almost but not quite a homozygous lot, and if one assumes that selection of extremes from homozygous population has no effect in shifting the mean, it would frequently happen that some individuals selected to continue the line would be homozygous in all factors and some heterozygous in one or more factors. The cause of the peculiar distribution of the population (high variability) of the F, generation grown in Bloom- THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII 24 0&8’ = $6'6 | 60° €0°E | STF SOE EPZ Z | 9 | ST| Z| SF) OS | SF| Z| ST/9 |Z IT [O |S | SIL T| si ays Te ZI61 Fi ¥-1-(Z-9) El = LIP | FO’ IS'T | $0" 1°62 |6EZ T |O |F | 06| 19| 02) #9} ZT1Z eee Cees (es Shae Beat Bee het ZE TI6I 44| 1-(-9) TZ'=8T'9 | 90° 06'T | 60°F 2°08 |903| Z | Z | OL] S| 98| GE) FH) GS/STIE |S IT Pp Topo og | OT6T | (Z-9) LT = 91'9 | FO" 6ST | 90° 897 |LOE)**)° "Tt EAD EE OS SO) 22 ee oe Obl Bit a 8% | 6061 | ‘A 9 ZZ = LO'9 | SO" EST | 80°F SSS IST || 110 0 10 11 0 [88198] IF PF OT! ti eT SZ | Ot6T | A (1-9) 61°+ 909| GO" ZEIT | LO’ TSS SEs 10 |0 |¥ [6 | | 2h) dL) 8h 108) FI Toc e | tter ta) Go 9g | og | 8 | 88 e | 18 | os 6z | 8% | 22 | 93 | 9z | FS | ez | zz | IZ | og | GT | ST | }ueTEG 3 JO | umoiyy Q “AO ‘a's $i = se = 30x F ‘ON que[q ied saatey Jo roquiny ana? 3 *NNOQ ‘CTHIANOOTgQ LY NMOLDY ‘9 ATIWV, NI SAVTI JO WAWOAN AO NOILAIIISIA ZONTAÙTHT IA Fay L 8T + F8'9 | GO" FST | LO°-F 06S | Z8Z | I |9 |sT|1e|s9|£9|zr|zeloz|ř |Z |T Eg ZIGI 84 €-1-(z-¢) LI'*G0°9 | GO’ ELT | 80°F 9°8Z| EFS | T | T|8 |0Z/ OF) S| HS) 2E/ Ste |S ZE TIGI 4a 1-(Z-9) SI'= GZS | SO’ 89'T | 80° 367} 92S | £ | F | ST|9S/ 9F/ 99| E| 9| L T | TE OT6I WT -¢ ST'= FES | FO'F OST | 90°F T87) P63 T |Z |ST| 68] S2/€2/29| 90/8 jg | T 8% 6061 sE g PE = SIL | 90°F I6'T | 60°F 99% TIZ TIT |9 |8 | 9T| | SF] So) 18) ZZi 0]|z GZ O16T I 1-3 9T'= ETS | POF OET | 90°F S'9Z| 6EZ cu T- |T /& |S 180 60i 12] $8) PE" ¥Z TI6I i I-(1-¢) UIV, is k 5 Poa | ve | se | ze | te | os 6g | 8% | LE | 98 | SB | FZ | 88 | Zz | IZ y Co uor nen E | quviq 19d səavag Jo requiny JoquinN A 9 ‘NNOO ‘GTaIaNWOOTG LV NMOWH ‘G ATINV,, NI SHAVATT AO UAAWOAN AO NOWMAaMIsIg AONTAÒTAA IA WTAViL 25 CHANGES PRODUCED BY SELECTION No. 565] *plyMoorg 4B UMOIH » IA’. o'ra OnT FI = L8'E | 90" = LE'I | L0'= 8°ZE |89T si ‘1g IT ege IO EEs Oe Oe SIGI | "a |cg-@-0) ST SOF | 90°F FST | 60°F TSS (ZF | T | S| 2 | St) 221 2h) 90919 Tt Ae ee SE TIGI LI g-(7-9) LZ°* ggg | 80°F OL'T | STF 8°08 BFO Tg |e oTi oioi Is) eiA eo baci Tana tame 0g OI6T | %@ (Z-9) ALT= 91'9 | POF 6G'T | 90° 8°SS |208" E SEES OT IT ET EGS E Gee ore 83 6061 sI 19 GZ = p09 | 90° = OET | 60°F 8'97 |€6 3l A "1S |8 |81103287 gi | gs O16T | PA (1-9) 6l°= I8'F | 90 F63 T| 201897 EST oT” ALAL OG TIEI 69) IE GiS ii a 9% TIGI 66 <33 = 33 Peart p ETE, Vgtd. T E Green Green Vgtd. Green Vgtd. Green Green pl 100 100-a a 25 75 100 ae Fs S aE pp ol >66 <33 66 33 no a h h | a | Be cr ye ee Gy VG i ee Mi 5 BA ARA Sas 78 o 100 oa A E Nd a a 28 T 100 100 Fic. 2. CORRENS’S DIAGRAM SHOWING THE INHERITANCE OF VARIEGATION AND LF-GREEN IN THE LEAVES OF Mirabilis jalapa Gd The diagram shows that a variegated branch of a varie- gated plant produces in F, mainly variegated plants, but occasionally a wholly green plant, while a green branch from the same plant produces in F, 25 per cent. varie- gated and 75 per cent. green plants. The F, variegated plants, however produced, behave in later generations just like the original variegated parent plant. The F, green plants, whether produced from green or variegated branches, are always of two sorts, namely, those that are homozygous and therefore breed true green, and those 3 Correns, C., Ber. Deutsch, Bot. Gesel., 28: 418-434, 1910. Der Uber- gang aus dem homozygotischen in einen heterozygotischen Zustand im selben Indiwiduum bei buntblattrigen und gestreiftbliihenden som ae eae * Numerals indicate approximate percentages; a— 0-10 per No. 566] INHERITANCE IN EARS OF MAIZE 91 that are heterozygous and therefore produce progenies of green and variegated individuals in a ratio of approxi- mately 3to1. Correns points out that a green branch of a variegated plant behaves as though it belonged not to a variegated plant at all, but to a hybrid between a varie- gated plant and a green one, in which green is dominant, and that half of the germ cells produced by the green branch carry a factor for green and the other half a factor for variegation. Similar results were secured from branches with self-colored flowers on plants with striped flowers, except that such branches produce few if any more self-colored plants than are produced by branches with striped flowers. Plants with self-colored flowers, no matter how they arise, behave as they would if they had occurred in an F, progeny of a cross of striped by self- colored plants. RESULTS oF EXPERIMENTS WITH MAIZE Hartley* in 1902 gave an account of an experiment with variegated maize. In a comparatively pure white strain, which occasionally produced a red ear, there was found an ear similar to some of the‘‘freak’’ ears noted earlier in this paper. Itis described as being red except for a spot covering about one fifth of the surface, in which the grains were white with fine red streaks. The excellent plate ac- companying the account, however, shows that most of the “‘red’’ grains had white streaks at the crown and that the cob was light-colored, not red. From the near-red grains of this ear there was produced a crop of 84 red ears and 86 pure white ones, while from the variegated grains of the same ear there came 39 light variegated ears and 36 white ones. Hartley refers to the parent ear as a ‘‘sport or sudden variation from the type’’ but does not indicate whether the ‘ in connection with 33 per cent. and 66 per cent. respectively, in his diagram, and gives no indication of how much less than 33 per cent. bred true or how much more than 66 per cent. segregated.) De Vries’s results with Antirrhinum yield readily to 110 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII the same analysis used with Zea and Mirabilis. Selfed seed from striped-flowered branches gave a small per cent.—from 2 to 10—of red-flowered plants. Only a few of the red-flowered plants were tested and these were found to yield 76 per cent. red to 24 per cent. striped. Selfed seed from red-flowered branches of striped-flow- ered plants yielded 71 per cent. red-flowered and 29 per cent. striped-flowered plants, approximating the 75 per cent. and 25 per cent. indicated by Correns’s results with Mirabilis. None of these red-flowered plants bred true, but only one test, and that of only a few plants, was made. The results were 84 per cent. red-flowered and 16 per cent. striped-flowered plants. It seems quite likely that had de Vries tested more red-flowered plants he would have ` found some of them to breed true. Correns’s results with striped and red flowers of Mirab- ilis differed in one important respect from his results with variegated and green plants of the same species, as well as from the principal results with Zea reported here and from de Vries’s results with striped-flowered and red- flowered forms of Antirrhinum. When _ red-flowered plants arose from striped-flowered varieties of Mirabilis, they behaved just as did the green plants that arose from variegated forms. But selfed seeds from wholly red- flowered branches of otherwise striped-flowered plants yielded little if any larger percentages of red-flowered plants than did selfed seeds from striped-flowered branches of the same plants. It would seem that in case of Mirabilis flowers, when the self pattern arises as ‘a somatic variation from the variegated pattern there is no corresponding change in the Mendelian factors for these patterns. In case of seed-sports from variegated-flow- ered to red-flowered plants, however, the factors for vari- egation are affected just as in case of green plants arising from variegated ones and of red-eared maize plants aris- ing from variegated-eared ones. The apparently non- inherited somatic variations of maize plants, noted briefly earlier in this paper, are possibly of the same nature as No. 566] INHERITANCE IN EARS OF MAIZE 111 the somatic variations in variegated flowers of Mirabilis. Some of these variations in maize are self-red cob patches on otherwise variegated cobs, and dark, variegated grains occurring in patches or scattered over light, variegated ears. GENERAL CONSIDERATIONS The experiments of de Vries, Correns, Hartley, and East and Hayes, as well as the records reported in this paper, all indicate that certain somatic variations are in- herited in strictly Mendelian fashion. All these somatic variations consist in the appearance of self-colors on plants that are normally variegated in pattern. The fact that variegated plants occasionally throw both bud-sports and seed-sports with self-colors is not, in general, to be taken as an indication that the variegated plants in ques- tion are heterozygous. Such behavior seems to be insep- arably associated with variegation. Correns has pointed out (loc. cit.) that variegated Mirabilis plants can not be considered mosaics of green and ‘‘chlorina’’ types due to heterozygosis, since they do not segregate into chlorina and green, but into variegated and green. The same rea- soning applies to variegation in the color of maize ears. Variegated-eared plants do not throw reds and whites, but reds and variegates. The conclusion seems irresistible that self-color occurring as a somatic variation is due to the change of a Mendelian factor for variegation into a factor for self-color. If this be granted, the behavior of these variations in later generations is a mere matter of simple Mendelian inheritance. From the title of his paper and the tone of his discus- Sion, it is clear that Correns regards, as the most signifi- cant feature of these inherited somatic variations, the change from a homozygous to a heterozygous condition. He even refers to them as cases of ‘‘vegetativen Bastar- dierung”’ or ‘‘autohybridization.’’ To me, however, the essential feature is the change of one Mendelian factor to another. The fact that this modification of genetic factors results in a change from homozygosis to heterozy- 112 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vow. XLVIII gosis seems wholly incidental. It follows from the circum- stance that usually only one of the two V factors of so- matic cells is modified. My own data do not in fact show that the change always affects only one of the factors at a time. While the results prove that this is true in a part of the cases at least, the F, ratios suggest the possibility of both factors being modified in some cases. It is of course utterly impossible at the present time to conceive of the cause or even of the nature of this change in factors from V to S. We can only conjecture at pres- ent as to whether the change may possibly be associated with changing metabolic processes in the maturing plant, or perhaps be connected in some way with changing ex- ternal influences, or even be a quality inherent in the V factor itself. It is perhaps significant that in maize, at least, the change, whatever its cause, occurs very rarely early in the life of the plant and apparently becomes in- creasingly more frequent as the plant matures. Wholly red ears in variegated-eared plants are extremely rare; large patches of red grains are somewhat less rare; indi- vidual red grains occur on most variegated ears; red stripes on the individual grains are very frequent, in fact all but universal in some strains, though in other strains —very light variegated ones—there may be only a few striped grains on a whole ear, the others being wholly colorless. As a matter of fact, even the presence of an ear with red pericarp throughout on a variegated-eared plant may not be good evidence that the change in factors occurred before the ear began to form. If the change took place before the ear was laid down, it would seem that the cob should always be self-red, since the red-eared progeny of such modified grains of the variegated parent plant invariably have red cobs, and cob and pericarp colors are coupled absolutely in later generations. But red ears, or nearly red ears, with light variegated instead of red cobs, have been found to occur as somatic variations on variegated-eared plants. Such behavior suggests that sometimes the factor change may occur almost simul- No. 566] INHERITANCE IN EARS OF MAIZE 113 taneously in the rudiments of every grain so that the grains become self-red while the cob remains variegated. We might, of course, account for the appearance of self- colored grains on a variegated cob on the basis of sepa- rate factors for cob and pericarp color!® by the assump- tion that one of these factors may be modified while the other remains unchanged. But we should then have the no less difficult problem of accounting for the universal appearance of red cobs with F, red ears without respect to whether the parent grains stood on red or variegated cops. Forced to its logical limit, our conception of the V fac: tor is that of a sort of temporary inhibitor, an inhibitor that sooner or later loses its power to inhibit color devel- opment, a power that once lost is ordinarily never re- gained. Ofcourse it may be that there is present in varie- gated maize merely a dominant factor for self-color, S, that is temporarily inactive, but that sooner or later becomes permanently active. Even if this be true, S as an active factor and 9 as an inactive factor are certainly as distinct in inheritance as they are in development and therefore deserve to be designated separately. And since in one case there results self-color and in the other variegation, the factors may as well be called S and V as anything else. It is of course also conceivable that the S factor may re- peatedly arise de novo, though this seems very unlikely. Whatever our conception of the nature of the factors for variegation and for self-color in maize ears, these factors are certainly as distinct in inheritance as any two factors could well be. Moreover, there is abundant evi- dence, which can not be given here, that they are strictly allelomorphie, as indeed they must necessarily be if one arises by modification of the other—this on the assump- tion that the factors are definitely localized in certain 10 Evidence that there are distinct factors for cob and pericarp color was Presented in a previous paper on coupling and allelomorphism in maize. Ann. Rpt. Nebr. Agr. Expt. Sta., 24: 59-90. 1911 11 This problem is discussed in another paper on the simultaneous modifi- cation of distinct Mendelian factors. AMER. NAT., 47: 633-636. 1913. 114 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vov. XLVIII chromosomes. Furthermore, these factors are to be re- garded as pattern factors. Though they must influence the development of the pigment in order to produce a pat- tern at all, they are now known to be distinct in inherit- ance from the factors for pigment—a fact that I have been able to show by use of a race of maize with a peculiar brown pericarp in addition to races with red pericarp. SuMMARY A somatic variation in maize is shown to be inherited in simple Mendelian fashion. The variation has to do with the development of a dark red pigment (or in one stock a brown pigment) in the pericarp of the grains, often associated with the development of an apparently similar pigment in the cob and husks. Plants in which this pigment has a variegated pattern may show any amount of red pericarp, including wholly self-red ears, large or small patches of self-red grains, scattered self-red grains, grains with a single stripe of red covering from perhaps nine tenths to one tenth of the surface, grains with several prominent stripes and those with a single minute streak, ears with most of the grains prominently striped and ears that are non-colored except for a single partly colored grain, and probably also plants with wholly self-red and others with wholly colorless ears. It is shown that the amount of pigment developed in the pericarp of variegated seeds bears a definite relation to the development of color in the progeny of such seeds. This relation is not such that seeds showing say nine tenths, one half, or one tenth red will produce or even tend to produce plants whose ears as a whole or whose indi- vidual grains are, respectively, nine tenths, one half, or one tenth red. Experimental results indicate rather that the more color in the pericarp of the seeds planted the more likely are they to produce plants with wholly self- red ears, and, correspondingly, the less likely to yield plants with variegated ears. Self-red ears thus produced are shown to behave in in- No. 566] INHERITANCE IN EARS OF MAIZE 115 heritance just as if they were hybrids between self-red and variegated races or between self-red and non-red races, the behavior in any given case depending upon whether the parent variegated ears were homozygous or heterozygous for variegated pericarp and whether they were self-pollinated or crossed with white. It is suggested that these results may be interpreted by the assumption that a genetic factor for variegation, V, is changed to a self-color factor, S, in a somatic cell. All pericarp cells directly descended from this modified cell will, it is assumed, develop color, and of the gametes aris- ing from such modified cells one half will carry the S factor and one half the V factor if only one of the two V factors of the somatic cells is changed, or all such gametes will carry S if both V factors are changed. The V factor is thought of as a sort of temporary, re- cessive inhibitor that sooner or later permanently loses its power to inhibit color development, becoming thereby an S factor. Or it may be that the dominant factor, S, is temporarily inactive, but sooner or later becomes per- manently active. Again, the S factor may repeatedly arise de novo. The cause of any such change in factors is beyond intelligent discussion at present. The results of Correns with Mirabilis and of de Vries with Antirrhinum are shown to be subject to the same analysis as that used to interpret the results secured with maize, ~ AL W N YN 3 {fe CELTE fiú EP malt l. "uye k l \\ Wihaareiitnn ih it \ ‘adoD 4abwnwo sninpsoydypg” 10 NOILVUOLSUWY - —, Pagik < y = -2 Sinua r Dep aps 7 fm eye ees a Sy ae h Dea AN AA zi a Se A $ : Wii “Yi ijt pt yHyhn Wik rafy WIH PI f R ge if, / H Y f XS g ` IIs Mee SP SELE Y | tol Wie yes SN 1 s n. ITN ` IIN MEA x N (ue ; a rA N : \* aa ae ` \\ Me \ yar MA i ` soy ei Zz i ay ee oe Y % r 4 RESTORATION OF EDAPHOSAURUS CRUCIGER COPE Proressor E. C. CASE UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN In the year 1882 Cope described from the Permian beds of Texas, an imperfect reptilian skull which he called Edaphosaurus pogonias. Two years later he described for the first time, the wonderful vertebra with elongate spines bearing lateral projections on the sides. These vertebr he assigned to the same genus as the skull but later they were removed to a separate genus as he con- sidered that the two specimens represented different forms of reptilian life. The vertebre with long spines and cross pieces were placed in the genus Naosaurus— ‘‘Ship-lizard,’’? a name suggested by the fancied resem- blance of the spines with their lateral projections to the masts and yard-arms of a full-rigged ship. From the time of the original description until 1907 the two genera were regarded as distinct but in that year Case’ suggested that the two genera should be united and that the skull described as Edaphosaurus by Cope be- longed with the vertebral column and limb bones de- Scribed under the name Naosaurus. The similar condi- tion of elongate spines, but without cross pieces, on the vertebre of the carnivorous genus Dimetrodon very nat- urally led to the belief that the two forms Edaphosaurus and Dimetrodon were similar in other parts of the body and Naosaurus merely exhibited something of the extrav- agance in spines, rugosities, tubercles, etc., which is such a common feature in the most highly specialized members of any group which is approaching the final stages of its family or generic life. The close relationship of the two genera was so probable that it was accepted by all paleon- * Publication 55, Carnegie Institution of Washington. 117 118 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII tologists and even Case was very reticent in his sugges- tion that they were much farther apart than was usually thought. Following the generally conceived idea of Nao- saurus a composite mount was prepared in the American Museum of Natural History in New York in which the skull and limb bones of a Dimetrodon were associated with the vertebral column of a Naosaurus. This restora- tion was published by Dr. Osborn in the Bulletin of the American Museum and a model of the creature in the flesh was prepared under his direction by Mr. Chas. Knight. Case in his ‘‘Revision of the Pelycosauria of North America’’ republished this restoration by Osborn but at the same time published an alternative restoration in which the skull described as Edaphosaurus was asso- ciated with the vertebral column of Naosawrus and the two genera were united under the former name, as it had priority. The composite restoration prepared at the American Museum has gained wide circulation in the text books but later discoveries have shown that it was unfortunate. In the summer of 1911 Dr. F. v. Huene, of Tübingen, while a guest of the joint expedition from the universities of Chicago and Michigan to the Permo-Carboniferous beds of New Mexico, discovered the remains of a skeleton of Edaphosaurus in which both the skull and a portion of the vertebral column were preserved. As the vertebre bore the typical cross-pieces of the genus Naosaurus the identity of the two genera was established but new evi- dence was speedily coming; Case in the summer of 1912 discovered in the Permo-Carboniferous beds of Archer County, Texas, the nearly perfect vertebral column of an Edaphosaurus (Naosaurus) cruciger Cope with the limb bones, and a crushed skull, identical with the skull origin- ally described as Edaphosaurus. From this skeleton, now preserved in the museum of the University of Michigan, the author has prepared the restoration shown in Fig. 1. The only conjectural parts are the size of the feet and the length of the tail; the re- No. 566] EDAPHOSAURUS CRUCIGER 119 mainder is based upon careful measurements from a single specimen. So far from being a carnivorous, rap- torial animal similar to Dimetrodon, Edaphosaurus was harmless, molluscivorous or insectivorous with possibly some ability to masticate vegetable matter. The edges of the jaws were lined with sharp conical teeth and upon the palate and the dentary bones were strong plates sup- porting numerous blunt, conical teeth. The head in all specimens recovered seems rather small for the size of the body and in this is peculiar in the Permo-Carbonif- erous reptilian fauna, in which the reverse is the rule. The shape of the head in the restoration is taken from the nearly perfect and undistorted skull in the museum of the University of Chicago. The elevated dorsal spines begin with the third vertebre and speedily reach a considerable height. The lateral projections are elongate at the base of the spine but above the middle are reduced to mere nodules irregularly arranged. The author is not in ac- cord with the suggestion made by Jaekel and Abel that the spines were separate, and can see no reason for the suggestion made by the former that the spines were mov- able. The strongly interlocking zygapophyses render such an idea impossible to any one familiar with the skeleton. Nor does the author believe that the spines were of any use to the creature as offensive or defensive weapons; rather, as he has frequently expressed himself, he believes that they were in the nature of excessive growths which may have had their inception and impetus in some useful function, but grew beyond that use as the animal became more specialized. The union of the spines into a thin dorsal fin is far more probable and the idea is supported by the presence of rugosities and the channels of small nutrient vessels such as would lie beneath a thick dermal covering. The anterior and posterior faces of the bases of the spines have sharp, low ridges which give place to Shallow grooves farther up the spine; only near the top are the spines similar on all sides. Moreover in the liv- ing genus Basiliscus, which has elevated dorsal spines, 120 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII and in the genera of the chameleons in which the same thing occurs, for example, Chameleo cristatus Stutch., the spines are united into a thin dorsal crest by the integu- ment and are further united by a thin membrane carry- ing scattered muscle fibers. The outline of the dorsal fin shown in the restoration is suggested by all the speci- mens in which the spines have been preserved. The sharp recurvature of the spines in the lumbar region is less pronounced in the specimen from which the restoration was drawn than in some other and it is possible that in other species there was even more of an overhang of the posterior end. The spines are abruptly shortened in the pelvic region and rapidly decrease on the tail. The length of the tail is not known but in all probability was elongate rather than short and stumpy. The limbs were short and heavy with the forearm and foreleg shorter than the proximal segment of the limb, a condition which is quite common in slow moving forms or those of aquatic or palustrial habit, and just the reverse of the condition found in the active, raptorial Dimetro- don. The bones of the feet have not been found in posi- tion, but in the great Brier Creek Bone-bed in Archer County, Texas, excavated by an expedition from the Uni- versity of Michigan in the summer of 1913, numerous large foot bones of a character different from those of Dimetrodon or the cotylosaur Diadectes were found as- sociated with the spines of Edaphosaurus and with large claws. It is believed that the foot of that animal was of goodly size and armed with sharp claws well fitted for digging in the soft earth or vegetation, tearing open rot- ten logs and overturning rocks in search of food. It has been noted by all collectors in the Texas beds that isolated vertebre of Edaphosaurus are among the most common fossils found but that any portion of an asso- ciated skeleton is extremely rare. This has led to the suggestion that the remains of the animals were trans- ported for some distance after death, probably by rivers from a higher land. No. 566] EDAPHOSAURUS CRUCIGER 121 Edaphosaurus was a highly specialized creature, slug- gish in movement and entirely harmless, living upon mol- luses, insects and perhaps vegetation. It probably lived in the woods or near swamps at some distance from the lowlands upon which were deposited the deltas which make up the Wichita and Clear Fork formations. In conclusion the author wishes to express his thanks to Dr. Ruthven, of the University of Michigan, for many valuable suggestions in arranging the pose and propor- tions of the restoration, and to Mr. Irwin Christman, of the American Museum, for the painstaking care with which his suggestions have been followed in making the drawing.? 2 A full account of the known specimens of Edaphosaurus and Naosaurus and a complete synonymy of the two genera will be found in Publications 55 and 181 of the Carnegie Institution of Washington. SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION HUMIDITY—A NEGLECTED FACTOR IN ENVIRON- MENTAL WORK AN admittedly rough but probably fair estimate of the relative interest which has been taken in the relation of the various environmental factors to insects, at least, may be made from the fact that Bachmetjew in his admirable compilation? of the work along these lines devotes, in round numbers, four hundred pages to temperature, one hudred and fifty to food and chemicals, seventy to light, forty-five to humidity, fifteen to electricity and magnetism and thirty to mechanical and other factors. Why is it that temperature is given about a third more attention than all the other factors put together? Is it true that it is nearly ten times as interesting or important as humidity ? A partial answer to the first question undoubtedly is that tem- perature is easily controlled as well as measured, whereas humid- ity, for example, is not easily controlled and the means of measuring humidity in small containers are untrustworthy and expensive. Furthermore, work with temperature gives results. The unfortunate part is that these results have usually been as- cribed wholly to temperature. In the course of some work at the Carnegie Station for Ex- perimental Evolution I found that I could change to a surprising extent the markings on the larve of a moth (Isia isabella) by @ varying the temperature at which they fed and moulted. How- ever, such changes were much more definite when the tempera- ture was kept constant and humidity varied. I did not have the necessary apparatus for getting accurate control of either factor, but I feel confident that temperature had little or no direct in- fluence. It was acting through its influence upon humidity. It would seem unnecessary to urge upon experimenters such a fundamental principle in the logic of cause and effect, but the fact is that with only two or three exceptions none of the more than a hundred papers having to do with the effect of tempera- ture upon insects tell us anything about the effect of temperature 1 ‘‘ Experimentelle Entomologische Studien vom physikalisch-chemischen Standpunkt aus.’’ Zweiter Band. Sophia, 1907. 122 No.566] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 123 per se. A few state that the atmosphere was ‘‘moist’’ or ‘‘dry,’’ but even then how moist or how dry is not usually mentioned unless it is believed to be saturated or absolutely free from moist- ure. It is clearly incumbent upon the one who makes such a criti- cism to show, either by his own work or in a review of that of others, that humidity is a factor of such importance that the criticism is worth the making—especially since the point is so self-evident and has been made in the past. The following notes are an attempt to justify the preceding. The experiments of many workers show that when lepidop- terous pup are subjected to abnormal temperature part, at least, of the adults which emerge differ from the normal. The observations have usually been made on color changes, and Fisher? especially has shown that warm conditions (36° to 41° C.) produce the same or similar effects as do cold conditions (0° to 10° C.), also that hot conditions (42° to 46° C.) produce effects which are similar to those produced by freezing (—20° to 0° C.). Fisher apparently had no means of successfully controlling the humidity but Tower? claims to have had this in his ‘‘ Investiga- tion of Evolution in Chrysomelid Beetles of the Genus Leptino- tarsa’’ and he obtained similar results, stating them as follows: The result produced by either a higher or a lower temperature is the development of a greater amount of pigmentation and a consequent me- lanie tendency in variations. This stimulus in both directions to inereased pigmentation reaches a maximum between 5° and 7° C. deviation from normal. Beyond these, as the temperature further deviates, there is a rapid fall in melanism, first to the normal, and then to a condition below normal, until a marked albinie tendency is found; and this de- crease in pigmentation continues until the zero point is reached, be- yond which no pigment whatever is produced. The zero point is reached much sooner, however, in high-temperature experiments than in low. Tower then gives the results of experiments in which all the environmental conditions, except humidity, are ‘‘normal.’’ Normal humidity for Leptinotarsa decemlineata is taken as rang- ing from 43 per cent. to saturation with an average of 74 per cent. The humidity in various experiments ranges from 10 per cent. to Saturation. The lowest natural humidity of which I have seen a record is 5 per cent. It occurred in Death Valley, California, *See Archiv fiir Rassen- und pense ST: -Biologie, 1907, IV, pp. 761- 793, for Fisher’s statement concerning criticisms of his co nelusi x Carnegie Institution of Washington, Pon aala No. 48, 1906. 124 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vow XLVIII where the monthly means for May to September inclusive varied from 20 per cent. to 27 per cent. The annual mean at Cairo, Egypt, is 56 per cent. and at Ghardaia (Algerian Sahara) is 50 per cent. at 7 A.M. and 26 per cent. at 1 p.m. The humidity at Buitenzorg, Java, during the height of the rainy season fluc- tuates between 70 per cent. and 97 per cent. during the day. Naturally, when dew is being deposited the humidity is practi- cally 100 per cent. It will be seen then that even Tower’s ex- treme averages (see below) are not beyond the range of possibility in nature, although they are as great as it is possible to use in experimental work, since at an average of 34 per cent. humidity only 0.4 per cent. of the larve reached the adult stage and atmosphere can not be kept supersaturated. The beetles were seriated according to an arbitrary scale in which ‘‘20 equals total melanism and 0 total albinism.’’ It is difficult to suggest a better method of measuring the extent of melanism than this, although we could wish for diagrams to aid us in grasping just what the scale means. I have tabulated the experiments and interpolated the normal data. Relative Humidity Per Cent. of Melanism Average Range Morjaiiiy Mode Range 100 100-100 90 4 9 95 82-100 30 7 3-11 84 55-100 15 12 7-16 74 43-100 ? 9 5-13 66 33-100 35 ll 6- 60 30-100 80 5 3-11 50 25-83 92 3 i 34 10-55 99.6 2 1—4. It will be seen that mortality increases rapidly as the humidity departs from normal but this can not account for the change in color since the range of melanism is doubled and in three of the experiments even the mode falls below the normal range. AS stated by the author: The results of experiments with deviations of humidity are almost exactly the same as those which were obtained from experiments with deviations of temperature. Such deviations from the normal either to- ward an increase or a decrease, produce up to a maximum increased pigmentation and a consequent melanie tendency, but beyond this the effect is reversed, pigmentation is retarded, and the tendency toward albinism becomes more and more pronounced as the deviation from the normal becomes greater. No. 566] bre car eon CRUCIGER™ 125 Alari on The point which concerns aa cm discussion is that not only does humidity have a definite regularly acting influence, but that its results are similar to those of temperature and, as with temperature, plus and minus variations of certain intensities bring about similar effects. If, as has usually happened, the hu- midity is not controlled in experimental work on the effect of temperature, how can it be said that the observed results are the effect of changes in temperature ? Tower made certain experiments in which both temperature and humidity are abnormal, normal average temperature being taken as 22.2° ©. Unfortunately, proof reading or something of the sort was faulty when it came to publication. Experiment 26 would be the most valuable for our present purpose, but the table includes records of relative humidity 35 and 39 per cent. above normal, 7. e., relative humidities of 109 and 113 per cent., respectively, if, as in the other experiments, 74 per cent. is ‘normal’? humidity. These are clearly impossible. The text figure illustrating this experiment does not help us since hu- midities are not given and furthermore the temperatures in the figure are rather consistently one degree different from those given in the table. Since there are two errors in text-figure 15, which illustrates the experiments with humidity as the only vari- able, it is likely that the figure is the thing that is at fault here. Several other similar discrepancies could be pointed out (as, for example, the temperatures in experiment 24, which concerns the combination effect of humidity and temperature) but it is prob- able that the author’s notebook records are correct and the tem- perature discrepancies in the published report are so slight that we may accept his conclusion. It is that when temperature and moisture are the variables in a given en- vironmental complex, the trend of general color modification is con- trolled by moisture (relative humidity), excepting in conditions where the temperature deviation is so excessive that the ordinary physiological and developmental processes are greatly inhibited. In experiments approximating natural environmental complexes, however, moisture is the dominant factor in influencing coloration. Even if there were no other reasons for urging the necessity of taking humidity into account, I feel that Tower’s work would be ample justification. Before taking up those reasons let us notice several cases where, on account of the striking results of the experiments, we must regret our lack of information as to the real cause or the relation of the several causes. 126 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII This same work of Tower is one of them. The effects just noted were merely ontogenetic. However, he made other experiments in which the effect seemed to be passed on by heredity. The fac- tors in the various experiments with L. decemlineata were 35°, 45 per cent. and low atmosphere pressure (p. 287) ; ‘‘hot, dry”? (p. 288); ‘‘hot, dry and low pressure’’ (p. 288); and ‘‘hot, moist’’ (p. 291), probably 31.2°, 94 per cent. Those with L. muliteniata were 30° and saturation (p. 292 and p. 293) ; and the one with L. undecemlineata was ‘‘10 C. above the average and a relative humidity of 40 per cent.’’ The work is of such im- portance because of its pioneer character that it would be un- gracious to complain too strongly, but the fact is that it is impossible to tell from the data given whether the effects are caused by humidity or by temperature or by a combination of the two. Bateson’s idea that there are no effects to be explained need not concern us here. There is a long series of interesting papers starting in 1895 by Fischer. As has already been mentioned, he finds that certain high temperature grades produce effects which are similar to those produced by certain low temperature grades. The eon- ditions of humidity are rarely mentioned, not to say considered. However, he occasionally confesses that they are important, as when he tells us* that it is necessary to have the warm air dry an the cold air moist in order to get similar forms of Vanessa by the application of moderate cold and moderate heat. I suspect hu- midity largely enters into the other experiments also for in one with high temperature,> which gave the same results as certain low temperatures and presumably high humidity he says the hu- midity was high. Like Tower’s experiments with beetles these concern color alone. Pictet® and Federley,” especially, have considered the effect of environmental factors upon the form of lepidopterous scales. Federley calls his work ‘‘Temperatur-experimente’’ and Pictet ‘‘Influence de 1’Humidité’’ but neither enables us to dif- ferentiate the effects of the two factors, although both obtained striking results. Kominsky* modified to a considerable extent 4 Algemeine Zeitschrift fiir Entomologie, VIII, p. 274, 1903. 5 Illustrierte Zeitschrift fiir Entomologie, IV, p. 134, 1899. 6 Mémoires de la Société de Physique et d’Histoire Naturelle de Genève, XXXV, Fase. 1, 1905. 7 Festschrift fiir Palmen, No. 16, Helsingfors, 1905. 8 Zool. Jahrbiicher. Abt. fiir Allg. Zool. und Physiologie, pp. 321-338, 1911. No.566] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 127 not only the color and form of scales but also the form of an- tennæ, legs and other body parts of Lepidoptera. He exposed the pupe to 42.5° C., humidity not given; 38° to 39° C. and 42° to 43° C., relative humidity 80; 8° C., “high humidity’’; 0° C., “‘very high humidity’’; — 7.5 to 5° C., relative humidity 80-90 and 50; and —11° C., humidity not given. For the most part the humidity was high and probably had much to do with the results, but we can not be certain. All the experiments just considered were made upon pupe. It should be remembered that only about one fourth of the weight of lepidopterous pupe consists of solids, and that the only way they can replace fluids lost by evaporation is by chemical changes in these solids. It is probable that they do so to some extent, although this has not been accurately determined. It is known that under normal conditions pupe lose in weight and the per- centage of solids increases. Naturally, a change in the humidity of the surrounding air would modify this physiological process and it is difficult to believe that it has not quite as much effect as changes in temperature, the humidity remaining the same. It is easy to see that, if the air is made more absorptive or less ab- sorptive either by the temperature changes themselves or by other means, and then the physiological activities are slowed or quick- ened by temperature changes, the effects will be much greater and might easily pass as due entirely to the temperature changes. The species which have wet and dry season forms in regions where the temperature is fairly constant throughout the year, as well as the tendency for the animals of moist regions to be mel- anic and of arid regions to be light colored, speak for the impor- tant influence of humidity. But there is another point in distribution to be considered. The study of distribution was long, and still is, largely an effort to get the ranges of animals and plants to fit isotherms. When yearly averages do not work, winter minima or summer maxima or accumulated temperatures are tried. The success which often attends these efforts shows that man is very ingenious and also that temperature is really one of the controlling factors, but it does not show that it is the only factor or, in fact, that it has any direct influence. The areas of grassland and forest in North America cut across isotherms as though they were merely political boundaries but Transeau® has shown that if we plot the ratio of temperature to °? AMER, Nat, XXXIX, pp. 875-889, 1905. 128 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vow XLVIII humidity we get a very close correspondence between distribution and climatice factors. Schimper’? has brought together a great deal of evidence which indicates that, as far as plants are con- cerned, even the major divisions of the world’s surface into arctic, temperate and tropical are fundamentally a question of the de- mand for and supply of water. Furthermore, if recent climatic changes have an effect upon the origin of new characters and the distribution of the organisms possessing certain characters, humidity is deserving of more attention than temperature, since practically the only evidence we have of such changes concerns humidity. It should not be forgotten that even aquatic organisms are subject to what amounts to changes in humidity. Peat bog plants take on many characteristics of a desert flora, although their roots are covered with water. It is water, however, which is not easily available, because of the chemicals which it carries. It is water which is physiologically dry. Finally, the great amount of work which has been done upon artificial parthenogenesis and related subjects is, in a way, a study of the influence of environmental factors. The obvious factors concerned have usually been various chemicals but at foundation humidity, in a broad sense, the addition or withdrawal of water by osmosis seems to be a factor of prime importance. Frank E. Lutz AMERICAN MUSEUM OF NATURAL History 10 ‘‘Plant Geography upon a Physiological Basis,’’ translated by W. R. Fischer. Oxford, 1903. VOL. XLVIII, NO. 567” fup MARCH, 1914 THE AMERICAN NAIURALISTI A MONTHLY JOURNAL Devoted to the Advancement of the Biological Sciences with Special Reference to the Factors of Evolution CONTENTS age The Effect of Extent of Distribution on Speciation. Asa C. CHANDLER - 129 - - i621 Biology of the Thysanoptera. Dr. A. FRANKLIN SHULL - Shorter Articles and Correspondence: The Endemic Mammals of the British Isles. ProfessorT.D.A.COCKERELL - - y - Notes and Literature: Swingle on Variation in F; Citrus — and the 185 Theory of Zygotaxis. Dr. ORLAND E. WHITE THE SCIENCE PRESS LANCASTER, PA. GARRISON, R. ¥. NEW YORE: SUB-STATION 84 Two Important Biological Journals The Journal of Genetics Edited by W. BATESON and R. C. PUNNETT A periodical for the publication of records of original research in her redity, variation, and allied topice. The JOURNAL also contains, from time to time, articles + eoniaasis zing > existing state of knowledge in the varions branches of genetics. e numbers are fully illustrated, and, aa the —— -matter demands it, colored plates are fears employed. The JOURNAL is issued in parts’ ae terial a mulates, and a volume (appeari ng, 80 tar as possible, anonkit A consists of four such parts. Bitederpélen price, $7.50 a volume; single copies, $2.5 Biometrika Edited by KARL PEARSON A journal for the statistical study of biological problems, appearing fate times a year. A volume se about 500 pages, with plates and tables, is issued annually. e X beginsin 1914. The nine preceding volumes form a veritable encyclopaedia of biological sons Ble a nike fields of painos eo" craniometry, medicine, heredity, etc. Subscription price, $7.50 a volume ; single copies, $2.5 Prices on back numbers of these journals vary, and postage from London is charged on back volume and seats copies. Published in America for the Cambridge University Press of England by The University of Chicago Press Chicago, Illinois University Control By J. MoKren Carrett, Professor of Psychology in Columbia University Together with a serics of Two Hundred and Ninety-nine Unsigned Letters by Leading Men of Scien? holding Academic Positions and Articles by Joseru Jastrow, Gores T. Lapp, Joun J. STEVENSON, > Camaron, J. McKean CATTELL, Gmorom M. STRATTON, STEWART PATON, Joun Jay CHAPMAN, JAMES Munnoz rr sao Gouin Boronu These ASEM and examples are taken from Professor Cattell’s informed and thorough discussion oie subject of pope control, a subject upon which À he has had much to say of late, finding occasion for ¢# eriticism of existing “an conditions, and standing as the peyare of an academic a and a tea ing profession upon which a man may enter without forfeiting his self-r —The Dial. Sentences and paragraphs that serge the expert, highly-trained mind, the suggestions that coms to te fresh a d tell us that a new day is abo ut to dawn in educatioual writing —The Boston Evening Transcript SCIENCE AND EDUCATION A series of volumes for the promotion of scientific research and educational prog” Ait VOLUME I. The Foundations of Science. By H. Porveané. Containing the authorized English translati by Gmoros Bruce Hausrep of “Science and Hypothesis,” “The Value of Science,” and “Scien and Metho -00 net. E. VOLUME II. Medical Research and Education. By Ricaazp M. Prance, Wruuam H. Werom, W- o Howser., P. Maru, LewrLLys F. Banker, CaarLms S. Minor, W. B. T r? Councttman, Tuzosatp Burra, G. N, Srawarr, C. M. Jackson, E. P. Lron, James B. HARRICE M. Dopson, C. R. Barden, W. Orntzs, S. J. Meurzer, Jauns Ewrwa, W. W. Keen, Haner H. som, CanmwrTIAN A. Herter, and Hanny P. Bowprros, Price, $3.00 net. VOLUME III. University Control. Now Ready. Pages 2+484. Price, $8.00 net. GARRISON, N. Y. THE SCIENCE PRESS LANCASTER, PA: SUB-STATION 84, NEW YORK CITY THE AMERICAN NATURALIST Vou. XLVIII March, 1914 No. 567 THE EFFECT OF EXTENT OF DISTRIBUTION ON SPECIATION ASA C. CHANDLER CONTENTS hac paea UE CLE Te ee et ae eneral Statement of Hypothesis.............eccseeecececceees 130 2. Tests of Theory by Comparison of Families............--++++ee0e 131 gle IR ee POET ME hoe, Oe" appr ran? 131 ON ida: vos wes sts dpa pa dad el Ghh E sek 133 3. Test of Theory by Comparison of Faunas of AreaS.............+++ 135 OBAE ogie caes ia epee eee 135 es oe eek as iiss T N 141 peel ak Airy Wii os i ose S «ee oN vide Vi ee ween Ones 143 ME i od ig ed nu dcp a a 145 Amphipoda (Marine Gammaridea)....... 147 4. Considerations Preliminary to Theoretical Explanation 148 sg sine SSE ees a A 148 Pee MN i CSU A Cee a a aa, 149 ifferentiation of Genera vs. Differentiation of Bpecies. .< 5 «seas 151 5. Theoretical Explanation of Hypothesis............ ....... 154 I. ao aa a A ... 156 en A O a a E 158 Waite engaged in some research work on the geo- graphie distribution of mammals under the supervision of Professor H. D. Reed at Cornell University in the fall of 1910 and the spring of 1911, certain conceptions re- garding the relation between extent of distribution and the generic and specific modifications of mammals were brought to light. Due to the valuable and helpful criti- cism of Professors C. A. Kofoid and J. C. Merriam, and Dr. J. Grinnell, and of other members of the University of California, and to the advice and aid of Professor C. A. Kofoid, the rather vague ideas then formed have been worked over and crystallized into their form as Presented in this paper. _ 129 130 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [VoL. XLVIII In the past, much of the work that has been done on zoogeography has dealt with a study of the facts of dis- tribution, both present and past, as they stand, together with a study of the factors influencing distribution and speculations regarding the explanation of some of the interesting and apparently anomalous facts thus brought to light. In all of this work, the distribution of animals has been considered almost entirely as the effect of cer- tain biological and geological causes. The present paper is intended to show that the distribution of animals is not only the effect of other causes, but is in itself the cause of other effects, and that extent of distribution has a direct influence on the modification and speciation of the group concerned. To find out how far-reaching and how potent is this effect, much further study is necessary, not only of the distribution of various groups, but of their classification and systematic relationships as well. In brief, the effect of extent of distribution on groups of different systematic rank may be stated as follows: As the range of a group of animals, be it genus, family, or order, is extended, the species increase out of proportion to the genera, the genera out of proportion to the families, and the families out of proportion to the orders. In other words, if we assume that in a distributional area of certain extent, there are three genera and six species, in a distributional area of twice that size, there will not be six genera and twelve species, but more probably only four or five genera, and twelve species; i. e., if in the first case the index of modification (a term here used to indi- cate the average number of species per genus) be two, in the second case it will be greater than two. As new distributional areas are added, other factors remaining equal, there is a constant increase in number of species and subspecies, going hand in hand with a diminishing rate of increase in genera, the result being a constantly larger index of modification as the area in- habited by a group of animals is extended. No. 567] EFFECT OF DISTRIBUTION ON SPECIATION 131 It should be remarked that a unit of area in this con- nection should be considered a distributional unit, not a geographical unit. In other words, while the addition of one hundred square miles might or might not involve a change in the life of a region, the addition of a new ‘‘life zone,’’ ‘‘fauna,’’ or association’? (see p. 155) would inevitably involve a biotic change, and therefore the addi- tion of one or several of any of these distributional areas should be considered as an addition of a unit, comparable to another unit of similar kind. Two possible ways of testing this hypothesis present themselves. We may compare the faunas of distribu- tional areas of dissimilar size, or we may compare the specific and generic differentiation found within families occupying areas of different extent. The former method we should expect to work out with a fair degree of accuracy, but the latter involves so many modifying cir- cumstances that even if sufficient data were at hand, it would be difficult to prove anything by it. In the first place there is the difficulty of comparing, in a distribu- tional sense, the areas occupied by different families, Since, as pointed out above, the geographic areas do not necessarily coincide at all with distributional areas; in the second place, while it is justifiable to compare the speciation of a family in one region with the speciation of the same family in another region, it is of doubtful value to compare the speciation of one family with that of another in the same or different regions, unless the other factors controlling their speciation be comparable or nearly so. In view of this there are few families which could be advantageously compared with each other as to Speciation in relation to extent of distribution, yet in the families which do seem to lend themselves to such a com- parison, the evidence all points towards the correctness of the law here proposed. The bats seem as favorable for such an interfamily comparison as any group of mammals that could be Selected, and the table (Table I) of their distribution by 132 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVII | TABLE I DISTRIBUTION AND SPECIATION OF FAMILIES OF CHIROPTERA Data Derived from Sclater and Sclater (1899) > | Ss | Index Family Distribution | Gen | Sp. of Mod V pa Se Pair eee E Aa T «oo bis va ee ues | 17 | 190 11.18 Emballonuride..... Warm parts of both hemispheres. ... . 15 79 5.27 Pletccddn. Shik ees Old Wordi eosl kk ES | 18 | 110 6.11 Rhinolophide...... Oe WOME Se ek ie bees eaves agers Us ea G 10.16 Nycteride......... Warm pate of Old World.......... Bee. p 7.50 Pil lipatormsdie. . . i NeDeranacel. 4 enar a FOE REE 8 | 2.25 families is significant. One family, the Vespertilionide, is cosmopolitan, inhabiting every zoologic region an every life zone, and it has 11.18 species per genus, the highest of any family of bats. The Phyllostomidæ, on the other hand, has the narrowest range, occupying only the warm zones of one zoologic region, namely, the neotropic, and has in 36 genera only 81 species, giving 2.25 as the TABLE II DISTRIBUTION AND SPECIATION OF FAMILIES OF INSECTIVORA Data Derived from Sclater and Sclater (1899) Family Distribution Gen. | Sp. of Mod. Sorickde. oo os. ose. Palearctic, rte poche and Nearctic regions, all zones........ 11 125 | 11.36 Erinaceidæ........ Palearctic, Ethiopian, ad Oriental i cece eal EES eee eee 2 16 8.00 TA. 3. Ss ss Palearctic Nearctic regions, tem- perate zo gag Oy Be neues re it 25 2.27 Wunaltde. . ooo oes tal ber mena gesa, Luwir 2 15 7.50 DOPE Ethiopian region, warm zones....... 3 17 5.66 Potamogalide..... C rear pots neg Sadana ICR SONNE tc no 2 3 1.50 Galeopithecide..... Maley only, y, forests, tropical zones. 1 2 2.00 hrysochloride....|South Africa. icase ikonia 1 7 7.00 entetide: . E ES A T E E E EE N TL n 3.00 lenod cehs end Bara a 1 | 2 2.00 index of modification. The other figures in this table are significant, but the indices of modification in the families Rhinolophide and Nycteride are abnormally large, and will probably be reduced by subsequent subdivision of genera, or discovery of new forms. able II shows the generic and specifie differentiation No. 567] EFFECT OF DISTRIBUTION ON SPECIATION 123 of the various families of Insectivores, but as some of the families have not been as intensively studied as others, and as the conditions affecting their distribution and speciation are so different in different families, we could hardly expect accurate results, and yet the table clearly shows a tendency for the families having wider ranges to have a higher index of modification, the almost cosmo- politan shrews, for instance, having 11.36 species per genus, and the families with restricted range (Galeopi- thecide, Solenodontide, Centetide and Potamogalide), having only 1 to 3 species per genus. The Talpide and Chrysochloride do not seem to conform in their speciation to what should be expected. When the specific and generic subdivisions of all the families of mammals have been worked out more per- fectly, and their ranges in a distributional sense, i. e., through life zones, faunas, and associations, are more accurately known, some interesting facts concérning the relation between their indices of modification, and the extent of their ranges, might be brought out. It is interesting to note that there is a considerable number of conspicuous examples of wide-ranging genera which are remarkably poor in species. Among carnivo- rous mammals there are many such cases, these animals seeming to be adaptable to an almost unlimited range of environmental conditions without modification, or, in other words, their germ plasm is not stimulated to change by altered conditions of climate or environment. The. tiger, for instance, is equally at home in the bleak frozen steppes of Siberia, or in the hot humid jungles of India. The genus Cynaelurus is widely distributed over the Ethiopian and Oriental regions, and yet it contains but a Single species, with several geographic races. Among birds there are a number of similar examples, the most striking case, perhaps, being Pandion, a cosmopolitan genus with but a single species. The same peculiar condi- tion occurs among lower animals, as for instance in the Dinoflagellate genus Diplopsalis, which is cosmopolitan 134 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII TABLE IIIA SPECIATION OF MAMMALS IN VARIOUS DISTRIBUTIONAL AREAS IN CALIFORNIA Data from Grinnell (19134A), (1908), Grinnell and Swarth (1913) Boreal and Upper Transition Zones San Jac. Mts. San Bern. Mts. Si R cow | BUNS | Stas | Soma Gen. Sp. Gen. | Sp. Gen. Sp. LAE TS A A O EN E 1 1 1 | 1 2 4 DOVOD 6 60s lees 1 1 ORCI yo ois oe ov 1 1 1 1 1 3 Antilocapridez...... Rodents...» TA 8 10 12 21 57 a ec: 4 4 5 5 6 22 in eee Aplodontide....... 1 1 Lit g 1 FAT VE 2 3 3 5 7 17 Geomyidez......... i 1 1 1 z 5 Saren AEE $ 1 1 2 n ee ee 1 2 Erethisontida eee 1 1 Ochotonide........ 1 3 ES UE 2 4 Carnivora......... 6 6 2 (7) 2 (8) 14 21 WOH... Coie hes 2 2 (2) ) 2 3 Canidae 6595's: 1 1 1 (2) 1 (2) 3 6 Mustelide......... 3 3 1 T 10 Procyonidæ........ (1) (1) 1 1 Uim: syne ces (1) (2) 1 1 Insectivora........ 2 2 2 2 4 11 POM se 1 1 1 1 2 q pe n E P 1 1 aS 1 2 4 Cheiroptera........ 2 3 2 3 4 T Phyllostomidæ..... Vespertilionide..... 2 3 2 3 4 7 Molosside......... COUR es acca 18 20 17 (22) | 20 (26) 45 100 Indices of modifica- ON kes ces peers 1.11 1.17 (1.81) 2.22 in warm and temperate seas, and yet is composed of not more than two species. No adequate explanation of these exceptional cases has been offered, and it is probable that their speciation, or lack of it, is due to conditions of their existence or constitution which we do not understand, or do not recognize. No. 567] EFFECT OF DISTRIBUTION ON SPECIATION 135 To test the law by comparison of faunas of areas of different extent, a series of tabular comparisons of the faunas of various regions of different size and character was made. In all of these tabulations, care has been taken in the choice of areas for comparison to make them of un- equal size from a distributional point of view, and to make them reasonably comparable. An arctic and a tropical region, for example, are not considered reason- ably comparable as regards number of genera and species, nor is a region on the outskirts of the range of a group considered comparable with a region near its center of distribution. Table III shows a comparison of the mammals of vari- ous parts of California. The regions compared are as follows: (A) the boreal and transition zones of (a) the San Jacinto Mountain range, (b) the San Bernardino Mountain range, and (c) the entire Sierra range, includ- ing the Warner and Shasta Mountains to the north, and the San Bernardinos and San Jacintos to the south; (B) a comparison of all the zones of (a) the San Jacinto Mountains with the immediately adjoining country, (b) the Sierra range as defined above, and including their foothills, and (c) the entire state. | A careful study of Table III brings out a number of interesting and significant facts, and bears out the law here proposed with unexpected accuracy, barring one seeming exception which, as we shall see later, can not truly be considered as such. Let us compare first the three areas in which only the two uppermost life zones are involved, and from which the species invading only the lower Transition zone have also been excluded. First, a word as to the areas com- pared. The Boreal and Transition zones of the Sierras take in over one half of all the representation of these zones within the whole state. These zones of the San Bernardino and San Jacinto mountain masses are, as compared with the entire range, very small indeed, and comprise almost as small areas as could justifiably be 136 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST TABLE IIIB (Data as above) (Data as in Table IIIA) [Vou. XLVIII All Zones San Jac. Mts. Si R Californi on (2,500 Sq. M.) (60,000 Sq. M.) (158,000 Sq. M.) Gen. | Sp. Gen. Sp. Gen, Sp. Ungulata. -...0...- 2 2 3 7 4 10 Bovis... 6 eek 1 1 1 2 1 y se NCEE AE 1 1 2 5 2 7 Antilocapride...... 1 1 Moecenue. ...is 25% 16 41 28 110 31 203 Betvide: anon 5 7 z 26 7 41 storim. so o 1 2 Aplodontide....... 1 1 1 2 vee eee ROE ae 5 14 10 33 11 64 eomyidæ......... 1 4 1 9 1 19 Héteromyidæ...... 3 12 4 24 4 48 TA TT OE T 1 2 1 5 Erethizontidæ...... 1 1 1 1 Ochotonide........ 1 3 1 3 Lepore. 11 3 18 Carnivora. ...-.:.. 9 10 15 29 17 51 Fade. a aso 2 2 2 3 2 6 Cepia. ia sk. 3 4 3 9 3 17 Mustelide......... 3 3 i 13 9 22 Procyonide........ 1 1 2 3 2 4 DV Or ee, 1 1 1 2 Insectivora........ 3 5 12 6 20 WE ras oa. 2 2 3 ' 8 4 14 Tee. -<.3c 1 1 2 4 2 6 Cheiroptera........ 4 7 7 12 11 26 Phyllostomide..... to 1 Vespertilionide..... 4 T 6 11 8 21 Molossidæ......... 1 1 2 4 TOL. es Sees 34 63 58 170 68 310 Indices of modifi- cation 1.85 2.93 4.56 considered to be individual faunal units. The San Jacin- tos are somewhat smaller than the San Bernardinos, but the difference is almost inconsiderable when compared with the Sierras. Before examining the table, let us see No. 567] EFFECT OF DISTRIBUTION ON SPECIATION 137 what conditions in number of genera and species would be expected in these three areas. The San Bernardinos, being almost as small a faunal unit as should be sepa- rately considered, we should expect to approach a mini- mum index of modification, i. e., a minimum number of species. per genus, approaching one as a limit. On the San Jacintos, these being smaller than the San Bernardi- nos, we should expect fewer types according to the law suggested by Grinnell and Swarth (1913), that the num- ber of persistent types in a disconnected area varies directly with the size of the area. On the entire Sierra range we should expect, due to the greatly increased territory, a considerable increase in genera, but a very much greater increase in species. Looking now at Table II, we find that with the single exception of the car- nivores on the San Bernardino Mountains, not one dis- crepancy exists. The Ungulates, Insectivores and bats are represented by the same numbers of genera and species on both of the small areas, and all of them show a marked increase in genera and species on the larger area, in every case with an increase in the index of modification. The rodents, which show a larger degree of differentia- tion than any of the other groups, show a very interesting advance in the index of modification as the area is ex- tended. The carnivores, as stated above, show a seeming discrepancy, inasmuch as there are six genera and six Species existing on the San Jacintos, and only two genera and two species on the San Bernardinos, whereas, if they conformed with our laws of distribution, we should expect at least six, and possibly seven or eight, species to be found there. On page 35 of Grinnell’s ‘‘Biota of the San Bernardino Mountains’? (1908) we find reference to a number of carnivores now rare or extinct on the San Bernardinos, which undoubtedly have been exterminated by man within the last fifty years. Counting these forms, which it seems to me we are justified in doing, the table bears out the law without a single exception, not only for S$ 138 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [VoL. XLVIII the total of mammalian forms, but the totals for each order and for each family. In comparing the three areas in which all the life zones are involved, the truth of the effect of extended distribu- tion on speciation is still more forcibly impressed upon us. In this case we are comparing areas which are suc- cessively larger in size, the San Jacintos, with their foot- hills and low passes involving the fauna of an area of about 2,500 square miles, the Sierras, about 60,000 square miles, and the whole state of California about 158,000 square miles. The following table, derived from Table III, is very significant in showing the diminishing in- Genera | Species Index of Modification Group | pas Jac.| Sier. Cal, SanJac.| Sier. Cal. |SanJac.| Sier. | Cal. i Ungulates.....| 2 3 4 2 7 10 1.00 | 2.33 | 2.50 Rodents. ..... 16 28 31 41 110 203 2.56 | 3.93 | 6.45 Carnivores... . 9 15 17 10 29 51 1.11 | 1.93 43.00 Insectivores. .. 3 5 6 3 12 20 1.00 | 2.40 3.33 i 4 7 11 T 12 26 1.75 1.71 : 2.36 TO. es 34 58 68 63 170 310 1.85 | 2.93 | 4.56 crease of genera, and the constantly increasing addition of species as the area is enlarged. By comparing the upper zones of the San Jacintos with the San Jacintos as a whole, and the upper zones of the Sierras with the Sierras as a whole (see Table III), we find that increasing the life zones has in a lesser degree the same effect as increasing the geographic area regardless of zones; in other words, adding life zones tends to have the same effect on speciation as adding faunas and associations without life zones. The follow- ing table (derived from Table III) illustrates this: San Jac. San Jac. Si Sierras Mammals (Upper Zones) | (All Zones) (Upper Benes) (All Zones) Genera Rl ERE UNM rie 18 34 45 58 Species 20 63 100 170 Index of mod : 1.11 1.85 2,22 2.93 Another rough test of the hypothesis was made in a comparison of the mammalian faunas of some of our No. 567] EFFECT OF DISTRIBUTION ON SPECIATION 139 large continental islands and zoologic regions, the results being shown in Table IV. The data used in this table are TABLE IV SPECIATION OF MAMMALS IN VARIOUS CONTINENTAL ISLANDS AND ZOOLOGIC REGIONS Data from Sclater and Sclater (1899) ( Africa ó ‘eee 2.017 000 Guinea Madagascar 11,770,00 947, 312,000 228,000 M. Group Sq. M.) ata Sq. M.) ea M.) ( Peg Sp. Gen. Sp. Gen. Sp. Gen. |Sp.| Gen.| Sp. Gen. Ungulates...| 155 35 1 1 Rodents..... 196 41 69 8 18} 5 13 T vores.. 59 22 9 7 Insectivores, 73 20 9 o PN Sa 101 19 83 26 39; 16|) 21 12 Lemurs..... 8 3 36 11 Primates T2 6 Hyraces..... | 14 1 Elephants... 1 1 Edentates 6 9 pi | 144 36 36) 14 Monotremes 5 3 3 2 | Totals. 2... | 685 | 128 301 73 | 169 59 | 96| 37| 100 47 Index of R | ification...) 5.35 4.12 | 2.86 2.59 2.13 by no means up to date, being taken from the summaries in Sclater and Sclater (1899), but the subsequent additions to the faunas of the places concerned, and the splitting up of genera and species, have probably been approxi- mately proportionate in each of the five areas, and there- fore the figures used are sufficiently accurate to be signifi- cant. Comparing Africa, the Australian region, Australia, New Guinea and Madagascar, which rank in size in the order given, we find that the indices of modification of their mammalian faunas are as follows: Africa 5.35, Australian region 4.12, Australia 2.86, New Guinea 2.59, and Madagascar 2.13. Certainly these figures are Significant. Comparing the mammalian faunas of the various islands of the Philippine Archipelago (Table V), we find that there is even here some corroboration of our law of 140 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST . [Vou. XLVIII TABLE V SPECIATION OF MAMMALS IN ISLANDS OF THE PHILIPPINE ARCHIPELAGO Data from Hollister (1912) Island ` | Sq. Miles Sp. Gen. Index of Mod. Reais oe aks cee | 40,969 72 40 1.80 UATE PVs Vy AG ne sa el 36,292 61 32 1.90 “EE rey Oo |` 5,031 16 13 1.23 TaS oe D E | 4,881 14 13 1.07 Founy oo acs 4,611 10 8 1.25 ee REO aa a Se She E | 4,027 21 18 1.16 Mindat., o oe. | 851 17 11 1.54 A N EUN S es 2,722 9 8 1.12 ir Sa wis bowie Ge koe | 1,762 8 7 1.14 ots a | 441 13 3 1.00 Maan us Rae Sak | 1,236 5 4 1.25 speciation. Considering the large element of chance in the animal population of a group of islands of such small size as those of the Philippines, where the various islets are at a varying distance from each other, and their faunas have originated from different sources, the rela- tion between their size and the differentiation of their forms is remarkably regular. In Table V, where the main islands have been listed in order of size, with their num- bers of genera and species of mammals, the deer have been excluded entirely, since their generic and specific differentiation is in too chaotic a state to be used. The most striking fact brought out by the table is the lead which the two large islands, Luzon and Mindanao, show, not only in total number of forms, but in index of modifi- cation as well. With the possible exception of Mindoro and Palawan, practically none of the smaller islands is supporting as large a variety of mammalian forms as could be expected of it, a fact which might be explained in a number of ways. In all of the tabafations i given, the marine mammals have been ‘entirely excluded since the factors affecting their distribution and speciation are so different from those of terrestrial mammals. In the majority of cases marine mammalian families have a paucity both of genera and species, a circumstance brought about by a number of factors. Generally speaking, large, wide-ranging } f No. 567] EFFECT OF DISTRIBUTION ON SPECIATION 141 forms, or forms which are poor in numbers of individ- uals, are poor in genera and species, possibly due to the comparative uniformity of their environment, which is usually coincident. Most marine mammals are of these kinds, and their paucity of types is emphasized by the comparative uniformity of their environment, even in the most FAITEN groups. From a distributional point of view, i. e., taking into account life zones, faunas and SA a cosmopolitan, oceanic, surface group of animals does not range through as great a variety of ecologic niches and environmental and climatic condi- tions as does a cosmopolitan terrestrial group. In order to determine whether the principles of distri- bution and differentiation here set forth would apply to birds as well as to mammals, a number of series of com- parisons was made as with mammals, and with exactly comparable results. TABLE VI SPECIATION OF BIRDS IN VARIOUS CALIFORNIA AREAS Data from Grinnell (1913B), (1908), Willett (1912) i California oe Pan bee hae tgs Sonan Pamei a (158,000 54 ie) - p Gen Sp. Gen Sp. ‘Gen, Sp. Passeres........... 62 82 79 114 87 197 a a E E 16 20 19 23 20 38 a A kk. 3 3 7 8 15 ceipitres. ........ 5 5 10 14 12 17 Columbe.......... 1 1 2 2 3 3 Moo fi 52 1 1 3 3 6 11 balie o. 3 3 4 4 9 10 OS a 1 1 5 6 6 8 Wades... 2 2 7 7 8 11 Anses... 2 2 5 5 11 11 Other water birds 1 1 12 14 16 26 coun Re 97 121 153 199 186 | 347 Index c ofr f mod... a. Ae 1.25 1.30 i 18 Table VI gives a comparison of genera and species of resident birds of (a) the San Bernardino Mountain region, (b) Southern California, and (c) California as a whole. Almost without exception, in each individual group of birds there is a reduction in the index of modi- 142 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII fication as the area is restricted from California to the Pacific Coast region of Southern California, and finally to the San Bernardino region. The totals reflect the trend in each group. While in the largest area the number of genera is considerably less than double what it is in the smallest, the number of species is more nearly tripled. The Southern California area is intermediate. TABLE VII SPECIATION OF RESIDENT BIRDS IN AUSTRALIA AND TASMANIA Data from North (1901-1909) Australia Tasmania ans (2,947,000 Sq. M.) (26,000 Sq. M.) Sp Gen Fam Sp. Gen Fam PRSROrOR: . 5. 3 os. 304 119 26 53 42 15 Wie Oe Pea eas eset cate e. 29 18 6 7 7 3 DE fee bee P A T eh 9 2 2 1 1 1 BOM UIIOR foe eo ie ke ees 27 17 2 11 9 2 AS T AT E E E se eee 57 14 3 11 9 3 ORME od ie ees se ee ss we 426 170 39 83 68 24 Index of generic mod Sah 4.35 2.83 Index of specific mod............... 2.30 1.22 | sie Table VII shows a comparison of the families, genera, and species of resident birds of Australia and Tasmania, from North (1901-1909). Here again, in addition to a very marked diminution of the total number of types in Tasmania as compared with Australia, each group shows a considerable decrease in the ratio of genera to families, namely, from 4.35 in Australia to 2.83 in Tasmania, and of species, to genera going from 2.30 in Australia to 1.22 in Tasmania. Table VIII is a similar comparison of (a) the resident birds of Ireland, from Hartert (1912), (b) the resident birds of all the British Isles, from Hartert (1912), (c) all the species of the Palaearctic region, the great majority of which are resident in one part or another, from Dresser (1902), (d) all the species of Japan, many of which are not resident, from Ogawa (1908), and (e) all the species of Kamtschatka, where the majority are resident, from No. 567] EFFECT OF DISTRIBUTION ON SPECIATION 148 TABLE VIII SPECIATION OF BIRDS IN VARIOUS PALEARCTIC REGIONS Data from Hartert (1912), Dresser (1902), Ogawa (1908) and Stejneger (1885) British Palearctic Kam- Ireland | “Isles. | Region | (1477o | tschatka Sq. M.) (120,900 | (19,150,000 | Sq. M.) (105,000 Group z Sq. M.) | 5q. M.) Sq. M.) Sp. | Gen. | Sp. | Gen. | Sp. | Gen. | Sp. | Gen. | Sp. | Gen. A S eee oe. 57 | 35 85 | 42 | 610; 116 | 180 | 64 | 55 | 38 APS es T 4 4 T 7 1 | 34 16 8 5 van MO O si 2 5 4 34 1i | 14 4 3 a E ES p 4 4 12 7 66: 31| 23) H | 15 7 CAMUIN T Sc si cc cs 4 2 4 2 2 0: 23 6 0 0 E eR re a) 4) 8) Ti 7p ed ALPS BS ET S T ee oe 10 9 15 1l 97; 32} 45) 21 25 17 sa, PEN i ieee ie AR 4 4 5 5 13 | 27 9 1 1 UU TT A se i 1 1 1 1 31; 1 23 12 0 PION ek: 8 8 16 | 12 24 89] 21; 28; W Other water birds...... 23 | 14 | 80| 15] 129) 35 32 | 18 Total aR e ee Wed wea aloe ae 124 | 87 | 188 | 113 |1,251) 310 | 491 | 204 | 183 | 112 index of mod... i- 1.42 1.66 4.00 2.40 1.63 Stejneger (1885). The increase in index of modification from Ireland to the British Isles, and then to the entire Palaearctic region, is almost exactly what should be ex- pected. The greater number of both genera and species in Japan as compared with Kamtschatka reflects the greater variety of ecologic niches in a warm country as compared with a cold one of comparable size. A com- parison of the resident species of Japan with the resident species of the British Isles would be of very great inter- est, but such a list of Japanese birds is not available. The very striking similarity between the speciation of birds in Kamtschatka, and that in the British Isles, both in num- ber of genera and of species, is very remarkable. The interesting manner in which the balance of nature is pre- Served is shown by the large representation of raptorial birds to parallel the abundance of shore birds and Anseres. That reptiles and amphibians are influenced in their Speciation by their distribution is indicated by Table IX, which shows a comparison of the genera and species of amphibians, lizards, and snakes, in three of the geo- graphic areas defined by Cope (1898). 144 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVII TABLE IX SPECIATION OF AMPHIBIA AND REPTILIA IN NORTH AMERICAN AREAS Data from Cope (1889), (1898) Lower California District) Western Sub-region | Medicolumbian oh (12,000 Sq. M.) (500,000 Sq. M.) (4,500,000 Sq. M. Index of Index of Index of Sp.| Gen. Mod. Sp. a Sp.| Gen. Mod. od | Mod. Amphibia... .. ee 8 1.25 23| 10 | 2.30 |130| 28 4.64 Lacertilia. .... 17| 13 1.30 |28| 13 2.15 |143| 31 4.61 Ophidia....... 16| 12 1.33 |20| 9 2.22 |191| 45 | 4.24 The ‘‘Lower California district’’ consists of only the tip of Lower California; the ‘‘ Western subregion’’ em- braces the Pacific slope of North America from Northern Mexico, east of the Sierras, to Oregon, where it crosses the Sierras to the Rocky Mountains, including northern Idaho, eastern Montana, and most of British Columbia. The ‘‘ Medicolumbian region’’ includes northern and cen- tral Mexico, and most of the United States and Canada north to a line drawn diagonally from New England to Alaska, interdigitating on its border with the ‘‘ Holarctic region.”’ The almost exactly parallel increase in the indices of modification in the three groups of cold-blooded verte- brates considered, as the area is extended, is quite remark- able. All three groups average from 1.25 to 1.33 species per genus in the smallest area, from 2.15 to 2.30 in the intermediate area, and from 4.24 to 4.64 in the largest area. As suggested by Professor Kofoid, a factor influencing speciation in such diverse vertebrates as mammals, birds, reptiles, and amphibians, should be very widely appli- eable to speciation in the entire animal kingdom. A series of statistics relating to various orders of in- sects and other invertebrates has been compiled to ascer- tain whether in these groups as well as in vertebrates, the number of species increases out of proportion to the genera, as the size of the area, in a distributional sense, is enlarged. No. 567] EFFECT OF DISTRIBUTION ON SPECIATION 145 TABLE X SPECIATION OF ELATERIDZ IN VARIOUS AREAS OF UNEQUAL SIZE Data from Schwarz (1906) Region Sq. Miles Sp. Gen. Index of Mod. “esis aay «eee 11,770,000 574 55 10.43 pep car Fuk care pe 228,000 245 36 6.80 nn Pe TSS NS oe 1,760,000 438 53 8.26 Peay cis Geneon 296,700 150 40 3.75 MUMIA ours 184,000 177 41 4.31 hemi ea ee ae 0,000 125 3.37 CMM Ss 6s si iors eae 25,333 96 28 3.42 eee 2,947,000 386 42 9.19 ETE ee 312,000 20 3.05 New Zealand.......... 104,750 137 24 5.70 T TETE 26, 7 1.86 Table X was compiled to show the number of genera and species of beetles of the family Elateride in various continents and islands, the regions chosen for comparison being well defined areas of unequal size A careful inspection of this table shows that with only two exceptions the indices of modification are directly proportional to the size of the areas. Borneo and New Guinea, however, not only show a smaller index of modi- fication than should be expected of them, but are poor in total number of types. Nevertheless, when we reflect that these two islands are not nearly so thoroughly known to science as are the other areas considered in the table, no great significance can be attached to their seeming paucity of known types. Table XI shows the number of genera and species of Limnophilide, a family of Trichoptera, in eastern North America (east of the Rockies) as compared with North America as a whole. It will be noticed that while in the TABLE XI SPECIATION OF LIMNOPHILIDÆ (TRICHOPTERA) IN NORTH AMERICA Data from Ulmer (1907) Region | Sq. Miles | Sp. | Gen. | Index of Mod. North America............ | 8,000,000 | 98 | 27 | 3.63 Eastern North America. . 5,000,000 45 20 2.25 146 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII larger area the number of species is more than double what it is in the smaller area, the increase in genera is only about one third, increasing the index of modification from 2.25 to 3,63. ~ Table XII shows practically the same thing in the case of the hawk moths of the family Sphingide. i TABLE XII ` SPECIATION OF SPHINGIDÆ IN AMERICAN AND AFRICAN AREAS Data from de Rothschild and Jordan (1907) Area Sq. Miles Sp. | Gen. | Index of Mod. Wek hd 8 ie a 76,000 61-| 20 3.05 Mexico kai Sai Dittik: 64s 975,200 | 122-| 34 3.58 Bouth Aois... o cs bie 7,000,000 | 197 | 35 5.62 Mex., Cent. roy anes. Amis: a S 7,975,200 | 237 | 40 - |. 5.92 Mex., Cent. Am., S. Am., and W. I.. 8,051,200 | 262 | 41 6.39 Bonbon. ési Si ews iv ies 965 7 5 1.40 MO ef on heed ee 8,000 9 20 1.95 By es ce ee ees Ge Ca iS eee 11,772,000 166 48 3.45 Afrion and Mad: ine neti Re eek vee 12,000,000 195 53 3.67 Afric: deny and Bourbon: os isini 12,000,965 197 53 ae 3.71 te this case iwo series of piaiations were made, one showing the number of genera and species in various Neotropical areas, and combinations of these areas, the other showing a similar tabulation for various Ethiopian areas, with similar combinations. It will be observed that the speciation in the West Indies is very large for the size of the area involved, but when we consider the abundant opportunity that has been given for isolation to operate, this is not surprising. The index of modification is quite low. Mexico and Central America have a larger specia- tion, compared with South America, than would normally be expected, the reason being that Central America is the American center of distribution. The index of modifica- tion, however, reflects the smaller size of the area, being considerably lower than that for South America. : The in- crease in index of modification from 5.62 to 6.39, as areas are successively added to South America, is significant. Looking now at the Ethiopian regions, we find that there is the same disproportionate increase of species over No. 567] EFFECT OF DISTRIBUTION ON-SPECIATION 147 genera in successively larger areas, the index of modifica- tion increasing from 1.40 in the small island of Bourbon to 1.95 in Madagascar, and 3.45 in Africa. Combining Africa and Madagascar, this is increased to 3.67, and with the island of Bourbon, to 3.71. Table XIII is one of especial sriterest; since it digas TABLE XIII SPECIATION OF MARINE GAMMARIDEA (AMPHIPODA) IN VARIOUS SEAS Data from Stebbing (1906) Area Sp. Gen. -| Index of Mod. M Mediterranean en ris ee a a ae 147 | 2.19 Arptic Godam i ai aa a eee a 311 | 140 2.22 ec Aono (JOUER. ky. 5c. i. hee ics ree 475| 176.|. 2.70 me Anau Ocak ia Gl AAS 44 | 1.47 Aroue and N. Allie oso os ces Be canes 588 |- 191 | 3.07 Arctic, N. Atlantic, so? S Atlante ok coe eek 645 | 207 3.11 Arctic, N. eae S. Atlantic pe Med. Bee st 735 | 214 | 3.43 Whee Gy 6a see 1,383 |.313 | 4.22 with a marine instead of a terrestrial group. It embodies the results of a compilation of the marine genera and species of Amphipoda of the suborder Gammaridea in a number of the oceans and seas of the world. Since it is primarily a cold-loving group, the largest numbers are found in the cold seas, the Arctic and North Atlantic being the home of considerably over half of the known marine species. It is very likely that when the Antarctic regions have been studied as thoroughly as the northern regions, the number of species from that part of the world will be very considerably increased. At the time of Stebbing’s work on Amphipoda, our knowledge of Antarctic ome contiguous areas was very meager. The steady inerease of the index of modification from the smaller to the larger seas is striking. The Mediter- ranean Sea, although it is the most thoroughly known of all, has the lowest index of modification, namely 2.19, the FOS Ocean -comes next with 2.22, and then the North Atlantic with 2.70. The small number of species from the South Atlantic and Antarctic regions has already been 148 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII mentioned, and its low index of modification may be at- tributed to the same sort of imperfect knowledge as in the ease of Borneo and New Guinea in Table X. The con- stant growth of the number of species per genus from 2.22 to 3.43 as the various seas and oceans are added to- gether, exactly parallels the results obtained in a similar way fora terrestrial group in Table XII. The comparison of the speciation of the largest area for which it was worked out, with the speciation of the entire group, many species and genera of which inhabit fresh water, is inter- esting, jumping as it does from 3.43 to 4.22. From the facts brought to light by this table it can hardly be doubted that practically the same influence is brought to bear on the speciation of marine as on terrestrial organisms by the extent of their distribution. © The theoretical explanation here proposed for this phe- nomenon involves a number of complex problems relating to evolution and speciation, including isolation, effect of time, causes of specific and generic modification, etc., each of which will be dealt with in the following pages as they seem to influence the law here proposed. Let us first consider the factor of isolation in relation to the production of new forms. As excellently stated by Cook (1909), isolation can not be considered as a cause or factor in evolution, since changes in the characters of species are not dependent upon the subdivision of species to form additional species. To quote from him: The separation of species into two or more parts allows the parts to become different, but there is every reason to believe that evolutionary changes of the same kind would take place if the species were not divided. That the isolated groups become different, does not indicate that isolation assists in the process of change. It gives the contrary indication that changes are restricted by isolation. If isolation did not confine the new characters to the group in which they arise, the groups would remain alike, instead of becoming different. . . . Isolation is the shears that splits the species, not the loom that weaves it. Therefore, while isolation can not be considered a factor in evolution, it is an important factor in speciation. Species vary in many directions or orthogenetically pro- No. 567] EFFECT OF DISTRIBUTION ON SPECIATION 149 gress in a definite direction, but the trend of variation or progression may be different in one locality, and tend towards a different result, from that of another locality. Whether the evolution, usually in more or less divergent directions, of segregated groups of individuals be looked upon (1) as the accumulation of numerous slight varia- tions which have a different average character in any two portions of a species, as originally explained by Darwin (1859, Chap. 4) or (2) purely as the result of natural selec- tion, as argued by Wallace (1858), or (3) as the result of a change in the average character of two portions due to the uneven occurrence of mutations in the two portions, a conclusion reached by Dewar and Finn (1909, p. 380), or (4) as the result of orthogenetic evolutionary tendencies inherent in the species and influenced by the environment, as Eimer suggested (1897, Chap. 1), does not concern us here,—the general tendency appears to be that two iso- lated portions of a species as a general rule trend in different directions, and diverge farther and farther as long as they are isolated. It is assumed that the greater the length of time given for the influence of isolation to be felt, the farther apart are the two originally identical divisions likely to trend, however the dissimilar evolution be interpreted. As stated by Tower (1906), in speaking of the method of evolution of the Chrysomelid genus Leptinotarsa, We can interpret the conditions found by any of the current hypotheses; but explaining a condition by an hypothesis is not the same as that the conditions found are evidence in support of an hypothesis, although it is often so used. The existence of distinct variations, subspecies, and ultimately species and genera, in isolated areas is a too frequently observed phenomenon to be looked upon as anything else than a self-evident truth, but that this should necessarily be considered as supporting any particular theory of evolution can not be argued. The profound results of prolonged isolation may be observed in the fauna of some of our long-separated con- 150 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII tinental islands, such as Madagascar, Australia and New Zealand. Decreasing degrees of isolation may be observed in our West Indian islands, where some generic differenti- ation has occurred; in the Santa Barbara islands, where there has been a differentiation of species; and the de- tached mountain ranges of Southern California, where the upper life zones are at present in an isolated condition, but have been so only long enough to develop a few new subspecies, and to lose many of the types of the mother range, in accordance with the law proposed by Grinnell and Swarth (1913) that ‘‘the smaller the disconnected area of a given zone, or distributional area of any other rank, the fewer the types which are persistent therein.’’ From this it is apparent that the time element, in con- junction with isolation, may have a very decided effect on the number of genera and species in a family, but since, from a geologic point of view, animals appear to have reached a new equilibrium very quickly after a geographic change, the time element may have little effect on the num- bers of genera and species relative to each other in any given area. In other words, as fast as new genera are produced in a given area, the species within the genera will tend to be produced in the same ratio, thus leaving the index of modification unaffected. As an example of the effect of time and isolation let us take a hypothetical case. Let us assume that a certain island became divided into two islands of unequal size, and that after a short period of segregation, just long enough for the fauna to readjust itself to the smaller areas and reach a new equilibrium, we had say six species in three genera on the larger island, and three of the same species in two of the genera on the smaller one. After a long period of isolation we should have approximately the same number of genera and species on the two islands, but they would have diverged to generic differentiation. In other words, the effect of time in conjunction with iso- lation is to increase the number of genera and species in the family, while the index of modification undergoes little change. No. 567] EFFECT OF DISTRIBUTION ON SPECIATION 161 This leads us to a consideration of the factors involved in the differentiation of genera as contrasted with the differentiation of species. In general.it may be said that extrinsic modifications, i. e., those which are in some way connected with changes in temperature, humidity, char- acter of flora, food, and other environmental conditions, and which usually affect such characters as color, size, length of hair, etc., lead to differentiation of species and subspecies primarily. On the other hand, intrinsic modi- fications, i. e., those which are related directly or indi- rectly to a change in the habits or mode of life. of the animal or the occupation of a new niche in nature, usually, if not always, lead to generic or family differentiation, since it is evident that changes fitting an animal to live arboreally instead of terrestrially, for instance, are of such a nature, that if they are perpetuated and carried to perfection, will not stop at specific difference but will become of generic importance. It might be argued that there are no modifications which might not, if carried far enough, ultimately lead to generic differentiation. This is possible, but very improbable, because the modifications here alluded to as ‘‘extrinsic’’ are of such a nature that in the varying climatic condi- tions there are likely to be intermediate forms which make the division of the more widely separated ones into genera impracticable. In the case of our ‘‘intrinsic’’ modifica- tions, intermediate forms are not so likely to exist when once the incipient changes leading to an altered mode of life have reached a fair degree of perfection. As a concrete example of what is meant by extrinsic and intrinsic modifications, let us take the squirrels of a given region, say eastern North America. There are four genera to be distinguished,—Sciurius, Tamias, Sciuro- pterus and Arctomys. The genus Sciurus contains Strictly arboreal, mostly nut-eating, omnivorous forms. Tamias includes forms which are terrestrial, diurnal, dwelling in natural or artificial holes and crevices, and with a device for carrying food in their cheeks. Sciuro- . 152 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII pterus is an arboreal type which is nocturnal, and has de- veloped characters which enable it more easily to travel from tree to tree. Arctomys is the most highly modified form, and has departed most widely in its habits; it is entirely terrestrial, seeks shelter in artificial burrows, eats grass, and hibernates. Were we to study the characters separating these gen- era, we should find that they are all characters which enable the animal best to occupy the ecologic niche it fills. If now we select any one of these genera and examine its species, we perceive that the differences we find are not such as could clearly be related to differences in mode of life or habits, but rather such differences as are induced by the circumstances mentioned above, such differences being size, color, length of feet and tail, texture of fur, etc.—t. e., extrinsic variations. An intorcating example of both extrinsic and intrinsic modifications in an incipient stage may be found in the song-sparrows of western United States. Let us compare the form of the humid northwest coast belt, Melospiza melodia morphna, with the form of the arid Arizona des- erts, M. m. fallax. The differences to be observed in color and size are very noticeable, and would undoubtedly lead to their separation into two distinct species were it not for the complete chain of intermediate forms. But even if the chain of intermediate forms were not complete, and after a period of segregation the numerous intergrading sub- species became broken up into a few well-marked species, nevertheless, unless a change in mode of life of the bird were involved, however far the extremes of color and size might tend, they could not be given generic distinction because of the intermediate forms, inhabiting semi-arid or semi-humid regions, which would be almost certain to exist. It hapens, however, that Melospiza melodia mor- phna, and M. m. fallax, do differ considerably in mode of life, the former being a beach comber, the latter a nomad of the desert. It would be expected, therefore, that if these two subspecies were isolated, the modifications re- No. 567] EFFECT OF DISTRIBUTION ON SPECIATION 153 lated to their difference in mode of life, already shown in an incipient manner, would soon lead to their generic differentiation. It is not argued that under a given set of ecologic con- ditions, only one type could be produced, nor that accord- ing to the idea of some zoologists, as set forth and refuted by Grinnell and Swarth (1913), should individuals of one geographic race be transplanted into the region of a dif- ferent geographic race, the first race would assume within a few generations all the characters of the second race. Whether the changes due to the influence of the environ- ment be looked upon as the results of natural selection and adaptation, or merely as the results of a stimulus to the germ plasm, the new type would not necessarily be always the same, this, however, depending upon the num- ber of potential responses in the type, and, as excellently shown by Ruthven (1909) in his study of evolution in the genus Thamnophis, upon the modifications previously undergone by the type we are dealing with. It is very evident that there are many variations in animals which seem to fall into neither the extrinsic nor intrinsic category, but which are neutral and vary inde- pendently of climate or habits, and may be inherited phy- logenetic tendencies. It is very largely due to these neutral variations, frequently to be ascribed to ortho- genetic evolution, tending in different directions in dif- ferent places, and given an opportunity to diverge by iso- lation, that different species may be produced to occupy regions of similar climatic and environmental conditions, and different genera may be found occupying the same ecologic niches. To choose an example in the same family quoted before, we may cite the case of Tamias in eastern North America, and Eutamias in western North America. In this case the characters separating the genera are not clearly related to their mode of life, the chief difference being the loss of one small premolar in Tamias, and its retention in Euta- mias. The extent of divergence of these neutral varia- 154 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII tions depends on the duration of geographic segregation, and may therefore be of specific, generic, family, or ordi- nal rank. To sum up, specific modifications may be of three kinds: (1) extrinsic modifications, induced by changes of climate and environmental conditions; (2) neutral modifications, due to a different trend of evolution in segregated regions; (3) incipient generic modifications. On the other hand, generic modification may be either intrinsic modifications, concomitant with changes in mode of life or habits of the animal, or neutral modifications as above, given generic value by a longer period of segregation. Having dwelt for some length on these preliminary con- siderations, let us now apply them to the case in hand and see how they affect differentiation into species and genera through extension of range. . It is a well-known biological fact that different types of a group of animals, at least of higher animals, are found associated with different environments; nearly related species do not, as a rule, live comfortably together in the same environment, and nearly related genera do not occupy the same ecologic niche in a given zoogeographical area. This does not seem to hold true for animals of lower organization, as conclusively shown by Kofoid (1907). It is common for a group of animals, unless hin- dered by an impassable barrier or unfavorable environ- mental conditions, not only to continually extend its range into new territory, but also to attempt to live in as many different niches in nature as possible within a given area. Such attempts to invade new ecologic niches are frequently concomitant with heritable modifications better fitting them to occupy their new situation, though it is difficult to say whether these modifications are causes or results of the change in mode of life. However this may be looked upon, the tendency to occupy new niches in nature is fre- quently accompanied by intrinsic modifications, and there- fore by generic differentiation. From this we may safely assume that in a given area No. 567] EFFECT OF DISTRIBUTION ON SPECIATION 155 a family of animals, by adaptive evolution, will approach a maximum of generic differentiation which can be sup- ported in that area. In other words, every suitable eco- logic niche which is represented in the region considered will be invaded by the family, and even in a small area there is likely to be a considerable generic differentiation, especially if isolation has had any opportunity to operate - within the area, in breaking up the genera and species. Let us assume that in one unit of area a certain family, Sciuridae for example, was represented by three genera, each with three species. Second, let us assume that this family kept spreading into additional units of area. With each new unit, the chance of new suitable ecologic niches being represented would decrease, and therefore the chance of new genera being represented would decrease, since if a genus were fitted for its niche in nature under certain conditions of climate and environment, it would in the majority of cases not be likely to undergo any radical changes in the occupation of the same niche under somewhat altered conditions of climate and environ- ment; i. e., the stimulus for intrinsic modification would be lacking. On the other hand, with each additional unit of area, the chances of the combined conditions of temperature, hu- midity, and environment being different, would remain the same. In other words, the chances of the three dimen- sions influencing the life of a region, i. e., ‘‘life zone” (controlled by temperature), ‘‘fauna’’ (controlled by hu- midity), and ‘‘association’’ (controlled by the effect of the other two plus a number of other environmental con- ditions), intersecting at the same point would be almost equally improbable with each succeeding unit of area. Since it is changes in “‘life zone,’’ ‘‘fauna,’’ or ‘‘associa- tion’’ which produce extrinsic changes, and therefore lead to differentiation of species and subspecies primarily, the increment of species would average nearly the same for each succeeding unit of area, other factors remaining equal. It should also be taken into consideration that 156 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (VoL. XLVIII with the invasion of new zoogeographic areas, contact with allied forms is frequently experienced, and oppor- tunity is thus afforded for cross breeding and hybridiza- tion, the result of which upon the germ plasm appears to be as influential in the production of new forms as is the shock of new environmental conditions. The constant increase in species and subspecies accompanying invasion of new territory, going hand in hand with a diminishing increase in genera, results in the constantly larger index of modification as the area inhabited by a group is extended. SuMMARY 1. Extent of distribution has a direct influence on the speciation of the group concerned in this way, that as the range of a group of animals is extended, the species in- crease out of proportion to the genera, the genera out of - proportion to the famliies, and the families out of pro- portion to the orders. 2. Comparison of different families having unequal geo- graphic ranges is usually inaccurate due to the great dif- ferences in the other factors controlling their speciation. Those families which do lend themselves to such a com- parison show decidedly the effect of extent of distribu- tion, e. g., the bats and some of the insectivores, the fami- lies of widest distribution having the largest indices of modification. A number of exceptions exist in the form of certain wide ranging genera which have a paucity of species. We have no adequate explanation for this phenomenon. 3. Comparison of the faunas of areas of different size gives very accurate results. A number of tabulations show as a whole an invariable increase in the index of modifica- tion as the distributional area is extended by the addition of either life zones, faunas, or associations. Such tabu- lar comparisons were made for all the classes of ter- restrial vertebrates, for several families of insects, and for the marine Amphipoda of the suborder Gammaridea. Allowing for explicable exceptions, the increase in number No. 567] EFFECT OF DISTRIBUTION ON SPECIATION 157 of lower systematic groups out of proportion to the in- crease of higher systematic groups as the area considered is enlarged is a remarkably constant and wide-spread phenomenon. 4, The theoretical explanation here proposed for this phenomenon involves a number of complex problems relating to evolution and speciation, including isolation, the time element, and causes of specific and generic modification. 5. Isolation is an important factor in speciation, since the separation of species into two or more parts allows the parts to become different. The degree of divergence of the segregated parts is largely dependent upon the duration of segregation. 6. Time, in conjunction with isolation and evolution, tends to increase the number of genera and species in a family, but the index of modification, i. e., the average number of species per genus, remains approximately the Same in a given area. 7. Three types of modifications in animals may be named :—first, ‘‘extrinsie’’ modifications, which are in- duced by climate and other environmental conditions, and which lead to differentiation of species and subspecies primarily; second, ‘‘intrinsic’’ modifications, which are concomitant with a change in habits or mode of life of the animal, due to the occupation of a new ecologic niche, and which usually lead to generic or family differentiation; and third, neutral modifications, which are merely the result of the natural tendency of all animals to vary and to be subject to more or less orthogenetic evolution,— modifications which can not be correlated with environ- mental conditions, nor with a change in mode of life of the animal, but which may be influenced largely by in- herited tendencies. Such modifications are responsible for the production, through isolation, of different species to live under the same climatic and environmental conditions, and of different genera to occupy the same ecologic niche. 8. Specific modifications may be of three kinds: (1) ex- 158 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII trinsic modifications, (2) neutral variations in segregated regions, (3) incipient generic modifications. Generic modi- fications may be (1) intrinsic modifications, or (2) neutral varations, given generic value by a longer period of segregation. 9. Since different. types of a group of animals are usually found associated with different environmental con- ditions or different ecologic niches, and since it is common for animals, if unhindered, not only to extend their range continually into new territory, but also to occupy new ecologic niches, and since these tendencies lead to specific and generic differsitingions: respectively, any given area will have a differentiation of species proportionate to its variety of environmental conditions, and of genera pro- portionate to its variety of suitable ecologic niches. 10. Since, as the area of distribution is extended, the chance of new conditions of climate and environment being represented remains approximately the same, the increase in number of species is nearly proportional to the increase in the area of distribution, but since the chance of new ecologic niches being represented in most cases constantly decreases, the increase in genera proceeds at an ever- diminishing rate. This, going hand in hand with the nearly constant increase in species or subspecies, results in a constantly increasing index of modification. LITERATURE CITED Cook, O. F. 1908. Evolution without Isolation. AM. NAT., 42, 727-731. Cope, E. D. 1889. The Batrachia of North America. Smithsonian Inst. Nation. Mus. Bull., 34, 1-525, 86 pls., 119 figs. in text. 1898, The Caiectilhada. Lizards and fechas of. North America. Smith- sonian Inst. Nation. Mus. Rep., 1898, 155-1270, 36 pls., 347 figs. arwin, Ch. ; 1859.. The pe of Species, 2d:ed., 1869. New York, D. Appleton & Co., , 440, 1 pl. Dewa, D; BP Finn, F. The Makitig of Species. London, John Lane Co., xix, 400, 15 pls. No.567] EFFECT OF DISTRIBUTION.ON SPECIATION 169 Dresser, H. E. + 1902.7 A Manual of Palearctic Birds, London, Vols, 1 and 2, 922, 2 pls. Eimer, Th. G. H. 1897. Die Entstehung der Arten auf Grund von -Vererbung erworbung Eigenschaften nach den Gesetzen organischen Wachsens, II. Leipzig, W. Engelmann, xvi, 513, 235 figs. in text. Grinnell, J. 1908. The san of the San Pree Mountains. Univ. Calif. Publ, Zool., 5, 1-170, pls. -19134.' A saei List Fs the Mammals of California, Proc. Calif. Acad. Sci., 4th series, 3, 265-390, pls. 15-16. 1913B. A Distributional List of the Birds of California. Mss. Grinnell, J., and Swarth, H. $S. n Account of the Birds and Mammals of the San Jacinto Area of Southern California, with Remarks upon t the Behavior of Geographic’ Races on the Margins of their Habitats. Univ. Calif. Publ. Zool., 10, 197-, pls. 6-10, figs. 1-3 (in press). Hartert, E., Jourdain AB geek R., Ticehurst, N. F., and Witherby, H. F. 1912. A Handlist of British Birds. London, " -Witherby & Co., XII, 237. Hollister, N. 1912. A List of the Mammals of the Argir Islands, Exclusive of the Cetacea. Philipp. J. Bci., D, 7, Kofoid, C. A. 1901. The Limitations of Isolation in the Origin of Species. Science, 25, 500-506. North, A, J. 1901-1909. Nests and Eggs of Birds Found Breeding in Australia = Tasmania. Austr. Mus., Sydney, Sp. Catalogue I, Vol. 1, 366, pls. A 1-8, B 1-7, ‘text figs.; Vol. 2, vii, 380, pls. A Hoe B 8-13. Ogawa, M. 1908, A Handlist of the Birds of Japan. Annot. Zool. Jap., Tokyo, 6, 337-420. de Rothschild, W., and Jordan, K. 1907, Lepidoptera, Fam. Sphingidæ. Genera Insectorum (ed. by Wyts- n, P.), 57, 157, 8 col. pls. — A. y 1909. A Contribution to the Theory of Orthogenesis. AM. NAT., 43, 09. 1906.” Coleoptera, Fam. Elateride. Genera Insectorum (ed. by Wytsman, 1906. Amphipoda, I. Gammaridea. Das Tierreich (ed. by F. E. eral Pein R. Friedländers Son, 21 xxxix, 806, numerous figs. in t 160 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII Stejneger, L. 1885. Results of Ornithological Explorations in the Commander Islands and in Kamtschatka. Smithsonian Inst. Nation. Mus, Bull., 29, 382, 8 Tower, W. L. 1906. An Investigation of Evolution in Chrysomelid Beetles of the Genus Leptinotarsa. Washington, Carn. Inst. Publ., 48, x, 321, 30 pls., 31 figs. in text. Ulmer, G. 1907. Trichoptera. Genera Insectorum (ed. by Wytsman, P.), 60, 259, 13 col. pls., 28 black pls. Wallace, A. R. 1858. On the Tendency of Varieties to Depart ee from the Original Type. J. Proc. Linn. Soc., London, 2. Willett, G. 1912. Birds of the Pacifice Slope of Southern California. Pac, Coast Avifauna, 7, 1-122. BIOLOGY OF THE THYSANOPTERA! DR. A. FRANKLIN SHULL UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN I. FACTORS GOVERNING LOCAL DISTRIBUTION INTRODUCTION Tue Thysanoptera, commonly called thrips, are only beginning to be known, in this country, by systematic entomologists. The systematic knowledge is mostly con- tained in the monograph of Hinds (1902), a more recent synopsis by Moulton (1911), and a few other papers deal- ing with new species and with relationships, prominent among which is the work of Jones (1912). Biologically the group is still less known. A considerable number of papers have been issued from experiment stations, de- scribing the life history (egg, larval, pupal and adult Stages) and habits of thrips of economic importance. Be- sides these the principal recent work of a biological nature is a paper of my own (Shull, 1911), on the ecology, method of locomotion, mode of reproduction, and dissemi- nation. The life cycle of mast species is still largely un- own. The first section of this paper is an attempt to carry into further detail the study of the ecology of the Thy- Sanoptera. The first ecological scheme, so far as I am aware, worked out for the Thysanoptera was that of Jor- dan (1888), who divided thrips into three classes: first, the flower-dwellers; second, the leaf-dwellers ; and third, all other thrips (for example, those living on fungi, under wet leaves, under bark of trees, on roots, on lichens, ete.). The inadequacy of this classification, and the difficulty of applying schemes of ecology adapted to other groups of sects, was pointed out in my earlier paper, where I pro- 1Contributions from the Zoological Laboratory of the University of > i No. 142 (Biological Station Series, Zoological Publication, 161 162 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIIL posed a new scheme, based on my observations in the field. In this scheme, Thysanoptera were divided into two groups: (1) interstitial species, those living in closely concealed situations, as among the florets of composite flowers, or in clusters of young leaves; and (2) super- ficial species, those living on exposed surfaces, for ex- ample, the surface of leaves. The interstitial species were further divided into an anthophilous division (flower-dwellers) and a phleophilous division (those living under bark seales on trees). The superficial spe- cies were either poephilous (on grass) or phyllophilous (on leaves of plants other than grasses). The distinction between poephilous and phyllophilous seemed warranted, since grass-dwellers were found on many different grasses, but rarely on other kinds of leaves. Such a classification undoubtedly describes the facts, but does not explain why the habitats named are the ones chosen(?). The factors determining habitat were be- lieved by me at that time to be character of food, and pro- tection afforded. In some species one of these factors predominated, in other species the other factor, while others may have been influenced largely by both. In the light of recent ecological studies, however, the explana- tion of local distribution in terms of such general environ- mental factors seems inadequate. Largely owing to the work of Shelford (1911) upon the tiger-beetles, much emphasis is now being placed upon the ecological impor- tance of physiological factors. With a view to relating the distribution of Thysanoptera to the physiology (more specificially, behavior) of the various species, and thus explaining that distribution in more definite terms, the experiments and observations recorded in this paper were made, This work was done largely at the University of Michi- gan Biological Station, at Douglas Lake, Michigan, sup- plemented by observations at Ann Arbor, Michigan, in Ohio and elsewhere. No. 567] BIOLOGY OF THE THYSANOPTERA 163 Facts To BE EXPLAINED The following are some of the facts of habits and dis- tribution of the more abundant species for which physio- logical explanations were sought. Some of these facts are stated in my former paper, some of them doubtless the common property of all thysanopterists; others, so far as I know, have never been recorded. Euthrips tritici is found almost exclusively in situa- tions where it is concealed, as among the florets of com- posite flowers, in clusters of young leaves, or in almost any close crevice where the tissues are not too hard or tough to be pierced. It appears to make little difference what species of plant is inhabited, provided a concealed situation is available. In the paper cited above (Shull, 1911) I gave a list of seventy species of plant on which Euthrips tritici was taken, and I have since collected it on a number of plants not included in that list. But with rare exceptions, it has been found in crevices where it was not readily visible. In related plants, it is always more abundant in those affording concealed situations. Thus, in white clover (Trifolium repens) and in red clover (T. pratense), this thrips is usually abundant; while on the related yellow, and white, sweet clovers (Melilotus officinalis and M. alba, respectively), growing along with the red and white clovers, Euthrips tritici is usually rare or wanting. The flowers of Melilotus are widely sepa- rated from one another on the stem, and do not afford concealment (Shull, 1911). If, while Euthrips is in one of these crevices, it is dis- turbed, as by gently rubbing or pressing the flower, it quickly comes out of its retreat and crawls rapidly away, or takes to flight. The larve show the same behavior as “ee “eae in this regard, except, of course, that they do n Anaphothrips striatus is found usually on grasses of various kinds, rarely on leaves of other plants. The spe- cies of grass seems to make little difference. Some indi- viduals are found in perfectly exposed situations, as on 164 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII the upper side of grass blades, others more or less con- cealed in the rolled up young leaves (Shull, 1911). I have found, however, that among the adults, those in exposed situations are almost exclusively females, while those in the rolled young leaves are either males or females. (For the first time on record, the males of this species, as will be shown in the second part of this paper, have been found in considerable numbers.) The larve, according to my observations, may be either exposed or concealed; the ex- posed ones are predominantly the older larve. In one of the grasses (Spartina michauxiana) on which Anaphothrips was found in abundance at Douglas Lake, Michigan, the leaves bear on the upper surface a set of fine, but prominent, ridges running parallel to the axis of the leaf. Adult females and larve of Anaphothrips on the exposed parts of these leaves were always lodged be- tween the tops of these ridges, and almost invariably with their heads toward the base of the leaf. If disturbed, they began to crawl along the crest of one of these ridges toward the base of the leaf. It was possible to force them to turn in the opposite direction, but if allowed to do so they soon turned again toward the base of the leaf, often continuing until they were among the rolled young leaves in the center of the top of the plant. Anthothrips verbasci is found exclusively on one spe- cies of plant, the common mullein (Verbascum thapsus). Furthermore, it is rare that a specimen of mullein, of con- siderable size, is found free from the mullein thrips. Most of the thrips are found among the florets or seed pods of the spike. Less commonly they are to be seen on exposed surfaces, as on the leaves or stem lower on the plant; but these exposed individuals are mostly adults. The larve are usually hidden on the flower spike unless that situa- tion is crowded by a large number of larve; and the larvé that are occasionally found exposed are mostly nearly fully grown. Anthothrips niger was not abundant enough during my stay at Douglas Lake that many observations of its No. 567] BIOLOGY OF THE THYSANOPTERA 165 habitat and behavior could be made. One fact, however, is of interest in connection with an experiment to be de- scribed. While the adults live mostly on flowers, some- times concealed, sometimes more or less exposed, the larve were always found concealed; moreover, it was with difficulty that the larve could be driven from their retreat by pressing the flowers. Frequently such vigor- ous squeezing was necessary to dislodge them that the larve emerging were injured; and a flower so treated was often found later to contain numerous dead larve. In this respect, the behavior of this species is in considerable contrast to that, for example, of Euthrips tritici. The habitats and behavior described above can be ‘‘ex- plained’’ in large measure if we say, as I at first proposed (1911), that certain species seek protection, or that cer- tain other species have specific food requirements. Thus, it might be said that Euthrips tritici seeks safety in crevices, and flees danger when disturbed; that Anapho- thrips striatus ‘‘prefers’’ grass for food, that it requires as much protection as its commissarial activities permit, and that its habitat and behavior are such as best fulfill these requirements. Anthothrips verbasci might be said to be limited to one article of diet, while protection is a minor matter. This explanation might be acceptable as far as it goes, were it not that no species is immune to attack. I have seen larve of Anthothrips verbasci frequently captured by various bugs. Heads of mullein where thrips are found nearly always bear bugs of the family Capside, and observations convince me that they prey almost wholly on the larve of the mullein thrips. The degree to which they check the thrips was tested experimentally as follows: Two mullein spikes of approximately equal size and equally infected with thrips were selected. The predatory bugs were removed from one of them, after Which the spike was enclosed in a thin muslin bag. Two weeks later the bag was removed. The enclosed spike re a large number of full-grown larve, a few had 166 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII pupated, and many were crawling on the inside of the bag. The spike that was exposed, on the other hand, bore but little over half the number of larve that were on the protected one, none were quite full grown, and none had pupated. Since nothing in the climatic conditions (heavy rains, for example) could have caused this difference, it is to be inferred that predatory bugs had devoured the larger larve in considerable numbers. Yet Anthothrips verbasci, according to my earlier ex- planation, ‘‘chooses’’ its habitat almost exclusively in re- lation to food, protection being a minor consideration. Can we not explain habitat and behavior in these in- sects in some way not implying choice, especially choice between conflicting preferences? May we not assume that certain elements of behavior are what they are without reference to their usefulness? If we grant the possibility of an affirmative answer to these questions, the experi- ments about to be described will have significance. EXPERIMENTS ON BEHAVIOR. The following experiments were designed to show the reaction of the commoner species of Thysanoptera to what seemed to me the most probable external agents affecting their distribution and behavior, namely, light, contact and gravity. Inasmuch as I was not primarily interested in how a given reaction was brought about, but only in its end result, the experiments were rather crude. Refinements were unnecessary, and their omission en- abled me to use much greater numbers of individuals than would otherwise have been possible. From ten to forty repetitions of each test were usually made. The experi- ments are described by species, only representative ex- periments being given. Euthrips tritici Light. Exp. 1—Adults of this species were placed in a glass tube about three feet long and one inch in diam- eter, closed at the ends with corks. One end of the tube No. 567] BIOLOGY OF THE THYSANOPTERA 167 was turned toward a small window, while the room was rather dimly lighted. All the thrips crawled rapidly toward the window. When the position of the tube was reversed, the thrips reversed their crawling, again going toward the window. The reaction was definite and in- variable. Exp. 3—A close-fitting sleeve of black building paper was slipped over one half of the glass tube used in experi- ment 1. The thrips were collected at the exposed end by turning that end for a few minutes toward the window. The covered end of the tube was then turned toward the window. The thrips crawled rapidly toward the light, until they reached the shadow of the sleeve. Here they crawled about, apparently aimlessly, for half an hour an inch or two within the sleeve or just outside it. Contact. Exp. 1—When, in the light experiments, the tube was reversed in position as soon as the thrips reached one end, the insects immediately turned toward the opposite end. But if the tube was allowed to rest for some time, the thrips became settled quietly between the glass and the sloping surface of the cork. The tube could then be carefully reversed, and most of the thrips re- mained lodged between cork and glass for many minutes, some of them for hours. The positive reaction to contact counteracted the positive reaction to light. “ep. 25.—A larva of this species was placed on a glass plate, upon which rested a microscope slide. When the larva in its crawling reached the slide, it came to rest in the angle formed by the glass plate and the edge of the slide. It remained there many minutes until disturbed. Gravity. Esp. 17—An adult female was placed in a glass tube which was enclosed in a black sleeve to exclude light, and the tube placed in a vertical position. The posi- tion of the thrips was marked with a wax pencil before putting on the sleeve. The sleeve was then removed mo- mentarily at frequent intervals, and the position and direction of crawling of the insect noted. Most fre- quently it was found lower than the previous position, 168 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vow. XLVIII and crawling downward. This was not always the case, however. Of other specimens tried, some showed positive geo- tropism more definitely, some less definitely than the one described. None showed a negative reaction in the ma- jority of cases. Anaphothrips striatus Light. Exps. 5 and 7.—Adults of this species were shaken out on a sheet of white paper near a window, and the course of their crawling was plotted as accurately as possible in my notes. Some individuals were decidedly negative to light, crawling directly away from the window every time they were tried, regardless of the direction in which they happened to be headed when they touched the paper. Others were indifferent to light, crawling in vari- ous directions. Most of the males used were decidedly negative to light, females usually indifferent. Exp. 10—Females taken from the exposed portions of leaves of Spartina michauxiana, and tested as above, were found in nearly every case to be indifferent to light. Females from the curled young leaves of the same plants were as a rule negative to light. Exp. 6.—Larve were usually found indifferent to light, regardless of whether they came from exposed or con- cealed situations. Exp. 15.—A single larva taken from the exposed part of a leaf, when placed in a glass tube one end of which was directed toward the window, crawled steadily toward the window. When the position of the tube was reversed, the larva at once reversed its direction. The tube was then placed in a black sleeve to exclude the light, and kept there for an hour. When it was removed, the larva showed for some minutes a decidedly negative reaction to light. Later, however, its behavior became indefinite, and soon became markedly positive. Darkness had ap- parently temporarily reversed its reaction. Contact. Exp. 22.—A female of this species which was No. 567] BIOLOGY OF THE THYSANOPTERA 169 negative to light was placed on a sheet of blotting paper. A small square of glass was placed over her, and sup- ported at one edge, so that in crawling away from the window the thrips approached the edge of the glass which was in contact with the paper. She soon became lightly wedged between the glass and the blotter, and came to rest. Blotter, thrips and glass were then carefully turned through 180 degrees so that the negative reaction to light would have led the thrips out of its crevice; but she remained there for a long time. Positive reaction to contact overcame the negative reaction to light. Another female, indifferent to light, was placed under a similar glass. In her random crawling she became wedged between the blotter and glass, and, notwithstand- ing that the blotter was occasionally turned in the mean- time, remained there several hours, until I lifted the glass. Another female, not negative to light, was placed under a similar glass square. She crawled from under it, but happened to crawl against the edge of the microscope slide that supported the glass cover. She settled quickly into the right angle formed by the slide and the blotter, and remained there a long time. Gravity. Exp. 21—A female which was indifferent to light was placed in a glass tube, and the tube set in a vertical position. The thrips immediately began to crawl downward. The tube was reversed, and the thrips im- mediately reversed its direction. A sleeve was placed over the tube to exclude the light, and frequently removed temporarily to observe the position of the thrips. In every case she was found crawling downward. When the tube was held in an oblique position, the re- sult was the same; the thrips crawled down the slope. If she was already crawling down, a slope of 5 to 10 degrees was found to be sufficient to keep her going in the same direction. But to reverse the direction of crawling, it was necessary to create a slope of about 45 degrees in the opposite direction. The same positive geotropism was Shown when the thrips was placed on an inclined sheet of 170 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII paper; but being here at liberty to fly, she soon inter- rupted the experiment. Numerous other females were tried, and all showed positive geotropism, some more promptly than others, but all perfectly definitely. A single male tested showed no definite reaction to gravity. A larva, nearly full grown, subjected to the same tests, showed as definite a positive reaction to gravity as did any of the females. With the possible exception of the males, therefore, Anaphothrips striatus is decidedly positive to gravity. Anthothrips verbasci Light. Exp. 4.—Adults of this species, shaken out on a paper near a window, crawled in various directions. None of them showed any definite reaction to light. Numerous larve, none of them over three fourths grown, crawled directly away from the window in every instance. Exp. 12.—In this experiment adults from concealed places in mullein spikes were compared with those from exposed situations. They were shaken out on a sheet of paper near a window, and the direction of crawling noted. In every case, those from concealed situations showed a fairly definite negative reaction to light. Of those from exposed situations, two were plainly negative, the re- maining ten indifferent to light. Exp. 11—Larve taken from concealment in a mullein spike were tested, on a sheet of paper, for their reaction to light. Those of the smaller sizes crawled directly away from the window. Those nearly full grown, while on the whole negative, crawled in a more or less devious path away from the window. One reddish larva, which from its color and size must have been nearly ready to pupate, was especially indefinite in its reaction to light. Contact. Exp. 18.—Larve of various sizes, which were found to be negative to light, were placed on a blotter under a square of glass supported at one edge, as de- seribed for Anaphothrips striatus. When, in crawling No. 567] BIOLOGY OF THE THYSANOPTERA 171 away from the window, they became wedged lightly be- tween glass and blotter, and came to rest, the blotter with all on it was turned through 180 degrees. The larve turned their bodies so that their heads were directed away from the window, but did not crawl away. The positive reaction to contact overcame the negative response to light. An adult tested in the same manner as the larve above described did not come to rest under the glass square. But happening to crawl against the microscope slide which supported the glass, the thrips came to rest in the right angle formed by the blotter and the edge of the slide, and remained there a long time. Gravity. Exps. 13 and 20.—Adults and larve were put, one at a time, into a glass tube, which was set in a vertical position, and covered with a black sleeve to ex- clude light. Some were examined at frequent intervals, others were left half an hour without examination. In every case the thrips were found almost precisely where they were put at the beginning of the experiment. This species is therefore indifferent to gravity. Anthothrips niger Light. Exp. 2—The red larve of this species were shaken out on a paper near a window, as described in other experiments. In every case the larva crawled away from the window for a few seconds at first, then slowly turned toward the window, and continued indefinitely toward the light. Once while the larva was crawling toward the light, I tapped the paper vigorously with a pencil, so that the thrips was lifted slightly from the paper and let drop; it immediately reversed its direction, crawling from the window, but in a few seconds turned again toward the light. The paper was jarred frequently, but always with the same result. To show whether the jarring made the response to light negative, or merely reversed whatever the larva was doing at the instant, the tapping was repeated at intervals of one or two seconds. 172 ‘THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII At the first tap, the larva, which had been crawling toward the window, immediately turned away from the light. Before it resumed its positive response to the light, the paper was tapped again; the negative response con- tinued. In this way the larva could be kept crawling away from the light indefinitely. Disturbance makes the reac- tion of the larva to light temporarily negative; otherwise it 1s positive. SUMMARY OF EXPERIMENTS Euthrips tritici, when disturbed, is positively photo- tropic in both larval and adult stages. It is positively stereotropic, and the stereotropism is stronger than pko- totropism, at least under certain circumstances. Some individuals appear to be on the whole positively geo- tropic; others are indifferent. Anaphothrips striatus Adult males are usually nega- tively phototropic. Females taken from exposed situa- tions are usually indifferent to light, those from concealed situations usually negative. The larve are usually in- different to light, regardless of the kind of place from which they are taken; a single larva that was positive was made negative by keeping it in the dark. Adults are posi- tively stereotropic. The females and larve are positively geotropic. Anthothrips verbasci—Adults taken from concealed situations are usually negatively phototropic, those from exposed places tend to be indifferent to light. The larve are all negatively phototropic, except the full-grown ones, which may be indifferent. The larve are plainly posi- tively stereotropic, the adults less plainly so, or not at all. Neither adult nor larva responds to gravity. INTERPRETATION OF THE EXPERIMENTS IN THEIR RELATION TO DISTRIBUTION AND BEHAVIOR oF THRIPS IN NATURE With the evidence from these experiments before us, may we not interpret the observed distribution and be- havior of the Thysanoptera in nature somewhat as fol- lows? Instead of explaining the fact that Euthrips tritici No. 567] BIOLOGY OF THE THYSANOPTERA 173 always lives in concealed situations as due to a demand for protection, we may assume that it is due to the strong positive stereotropism of this species—aided in some cases by positive geotropism, where the flower inhabited is upright, but notwithstanding positive geotropism where the flower is inverted. The rapid escape by crawl- ing or flight when disturbed is not due to the fact that this is the best way of avoiding danger, but to the posi- tive reaction to light. Other species avoid danger by going deeper into crevices, because they are negatively responsive to light. Anaphothrips striatus lives on grasses doubtless be- cause it can not live on any other food, or because the reproductive processes are not stimulated by any other host plant. But their distribution and behavior on the grasses may be explained largely in terms of their reac- tions to the three agents tested in the experiments. The males usually live in concealed situations on the plants (curled-up leaves) because they are mostly negatively phototropic, and crawl down the leaves until they reach these concealed situations. Females may live either in exposed or in concealed places, for some of them are negative to light, others indifferent. The larvæ are either exposed or concealed, because they are indifferent to light. The eggs from which they hatch are probably laid by negatively phototropic females in the young curled leaves, and the leaves unfold as the larvæ develop; this explains why the exposed larvæ are much larger, on the average, than are those concealed in the young leaves. Perhaps the relation of cause and effect as here stated is reversed, at least for some cases. Concealment—caused in one way or another—may lead to negative phototro- pism, as in the larva which was made temporarily nega- tively phototropic by being kept in the dark. The adults are lodged between the ridges on the upper side of the leaves of the grass Spartina, not for the sake of protection, it seems to me, but because they are posi- tively stereotropic. Doubtless between the ridges is the 174 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVI place where they can best suck the juices of the plant, but there is no need to assume that they deliberately choose this location in order to get their food most easily. Both adults and larve rest on these leaves with their heads directed toward the base of the leaf, and crawl toward the base of the leaf if disturbed, not because protection is most quickly to be found among the curled leaves at the center of the plant, but because the thrips are positively geotropic. Anthothrips verbasci—The larve of this species live hidden among the flowers of the mullein spike, not be- cause they must get their food there, for they can get it from any part of the plant; nor do they hide there, it seems to me, to secure protection. They remain in these crevices because, excepting the largest larve, they are positively stereotropic and negatively phototropic. The adults are sometimes exposed, sometimes concealed, prob- ably because in the former case they are usually indiffer- ent to light, in the latter case negatively phototropic. (Or may they be made negative or indifferent according as they live—for one reason or another—concealed or ex- posed?) , Thus, while Anthothrips verbasci is limited to one food plant, and the food requirements are therefore probably exceedingly important, yet the distribution and behavior of the insects on this plant may be explained without ap- pealing to anything like ‘‘choice’’ in other matters. Regarding Anthothrips niger, I wish to call attention to but one fact. The difficulty with which the larve are driven forth from a flower in which they live appears to be due, not to a persistent attempt at concealment, but to the fact that on being disturbed they are temporarily negatively phototropic; if the disturbance is continued, the negative response continues. The only argument which, it appears to me, could be advanced in favor of assuming ‘that the Thysanoptera choose their locations, instead of adopting simple re- sponse to external stimuli as the correct explanation of No. 567] BIOLOGY OF THE THYSANOPTERA 175 distribution, is the possibility that they have learned that certain modes of behavior are best suited (for example) to continued safety. The reply to such an argument is first, that most of my studies on behavior have been made in an unsettled re- gion, where the enemies of thrips incident to civilization are practically wanting, and where even the natural enemies are not abundant. It could hardly be assumed that every individual would learn to avoid its enemies in the course of its short lifetime, yet certain species seem to be invariable in their response to certain agents. Furthermore, many of the larve tested in the experi- ments could have been but a few days old. It is incred- ible that their reactions should have been, as in fact they were, as definite and invariable as those of older larve, if these responses were dependent on experience. It seems to me, therefore, that the only satisfactory ex- planation of outdoor behavior and distribution of the Thysanoptera lies in the assumption that they are in large measure the result of responses to simple stimuli, and do not imply any degree of choice. ORIGIN AND ÅDAPTIVENESS or RESPONSES TO EXTERNAL STIMULI The origin of such responses in Thysanoptera as have been described above is not, I believe, discoverable. Pur- poseful they most probably are not, as I have shown, if by purpose we mean conscious direction of actions to some end. But adaptive they no doubt are in many cases. Perhaps they are all adaptive, but I confess that my powers of analysis are not keen enough to prove such a view correct. That Euthrips tritici is positively photo- tropic when disturbed is no doubt the cause of frequent escapes from danger. One may even believe the negative Phototropism of larvæ of Anthothrips verbasci to be adaptive, because they are much more sluggish than is Euthrips tritici, and could not escape quickly even if they should emerge into the light. They are probably safest, 176 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII therefore, if, when disturbed, they retire into still deeper crevices. But I am unable to discover the adaptiveness of the response of the larve of Anthothrips niger to light —at first negative, on being disturbed, but soon becoming positive. Nor can I understand why the males of Ana- phothrips striatus are more definitely negative to light than are the females or larve. These reactions seem to me to be useless. We need not demand that all of these responses be adaptive, any more than that they be purposeful. Re- sponses have arisen, no one knows how. They have been preserved, and we can but speculate as to the method of their preservation. Natural selection may be respon- sible for the preservation of the useful, and it may have eliminated responses that were harmful. But other re- sponses of no value whatever, but likewise harmless, may have been allowed to persist, without help or hindrance from selection. (To be continued.) SHORTER ARTICLES AND CORRESPONDENCE THE ENDEMIC MAMMALS OF THE BRITISH ISLANDS WHEN, in 1891, I was collecting information to be used by Dr. A. R. Wallace in preparing the second edition of his ‘‘ Island Life,” I found much skepticism among naturalists concerning the alleged endemic or precinctive elements of the British fauna. Dr. Wallace was able to give lists of supposed precinctive species and varieties belonging to several groups, but for the mammals he was obliged to state, ‘‘it is the opinion of the best authorities that we possess neither a distinct species nor distinguishable variety.” -We little imagined that about twenty years later the British Museum would issue a work describing ten species and twenty subspecies of mammals peculiar to the British Islands; twenty-one of these being actually undescribed at the time I made my enquiries, and the rest then reposing quietly in the Synonymy. Still less did we imagine that such a revision, when made, would be the work of an American, coming over from the United States National Museum to show Europeans the neglected wonders of their own fauna! The Catalogue of the Mammals of Western Europe, by Mr. G. S. Miller, published last year by the British Museum, is certainly one of the most remarkable zoolog- ical works ever produced, and is well worthy of the attention of all naturalists, whether specially interested in the Mammalia or not. While so many students of genetics are giving us the results of their experiments in breeding mammals, it is worth while to turn also to the results of nature’s long-time breeding experiments, so clearly set forth by Mr. Miller in the volume cited. What, after all, is the connection between the phenomena seen by the breeder and the facts of mammalian evolution? Do species and subspecies differ by ‘‘units,’’ and do the variations observed in captivity correspond in any way to the recorded specifie and subspecifie differences? A complete analysis of Mr. Miller’s volume can not be made at the present time, but I have extracted the list, given below, of the forms supposed to be confined to the British Islands, giving their distribution and principal. distinctive characters. I have added to Mr. Miller’s list three quite recently described animals. On examining the list, it appears that a few of the species must belong to the older fauna of the country, not wholly exterminated by the glacial ice and periods of partial submergence. Such are 177 178 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII Mustela hibernica of Ireland and Microtus orcadensis of the Orkney Islands. It is at least suggestive, in this connection, that so many of the Scottish islands yield animals differing from those of the mainland. In the majority of cases, however, the peculiar British mammals are closely related to those of the con- tinent, and might well be of very recent origin. There is a decided tendency to darker colors, such as has been noted also among British moths. In spite of this tendency, however, some forms are lighter than their relatives, the most conspicuous case being the light-tailed British squirrel. In several cases the differ- ence noted has in part to do with particular phases; thus the squirrel has no dark phase, and the ermine does not turn so white in winter. The British red grouse, it will be remembered, is peculiar in lacking a white winter phase. Some of these differ- ences may be due to the direct effect of the mild and moist British climate, and would perhaps disappear in the descendants of British animals taken elsewhere. The experiments on birds by Beebe are very suggestive in this connection. In other cases, the distinctions are such as might readily result from changes in one or two ‘‘units,’’ such as are observed in experimental breeding. When we have a variable type, subject to losses and new combi- nations of unit characters, it is perhaps to be expected that different groups of individuals, isolated from one another, will after a time produce different homozygous combinations. That is to say, the result comes from a long series of ‘‘accidents,”’ which will probably not be duplicated in two different places. In this way mere isolation may be an adequate cause of modification, providing always that through variation degrees of hetero- zygosity have arisen. In the common house mouse, Mus musculus, Hagedoorn’ has isolated and figured a great number of color varieties, for nearly all of which he has constructed zygotic formule. Little? has also described and figured a similar series of varieties, appar- ently in ignorance of Hagedoorn’s paper, which he does not cite. He gives zygotic formule for thirty-two different varieties, but not all of them are visibly different. Albino varieties, resulting from the dropping out of a particular determiner, may be pro- duced, corresponding in other respects to each of the thirty-two colored forms, although they all look alike, and will only show their true characters on crossing. Several of the varieties show 1 Zeit. f. ind. Abst. Ver., 1912. 2 ‘í Experimental Studies of the Inheritance of Color in Mice,’’ 1913. No. 567]. SHORTER ARTICLES AND CORRESPONDENCE 179 noteworthy fluctuating variability, due to differences in ex- pression, Mus musculus, then, is very conspicuously variable in color; yet Miller’s book records only one subspecies, that of the Medi- terranean region and the Azores, which is less dusky and more yellowish, with the under parts buffy grayish. It possibly agrees with Little’s ‘‘dilute black agouti’’ variety. On the other hand, M. musculus has a recognized subspecies in Mexico, where it must have developed since the species was introduced by man. The mice of St. Kilda and the Faroe Islands, although given as distinct species, are derivatives of Mus musculus, differing in other points than color. In connection with subspecifie differ- ences in size, Sumner’s experiments with different temperatures should be noted, since they prove that differences of temperature might lead to readily measurable differences in dimensions, wholly unconnected with losses of determiners or new zygotic combinations. Whether or not diverse conditions of this sort would ultimately affect the germ plasm, their effects would be patent long before and quite independently of any such modifi- cation. On the whole, the poverty of Mus musculus in subspecies would suggest that the variations observed by breeders are not, as a rule, the stuff that new subspecies are made of. Against this argument may well be adduced the fact that M. musculus is an urban animal, constantly traveling about, so that incipient races do not remain isolated. Here the closely related rats, Epimys, are worth considering. For Europe Miller can only recognize the Norway, Black and Alexandrian rats, all widespread, prac- tically cosmopolitan. Yet in the Malay Archipelago, where Epimys is distributed over myriads of islands, large and small, the species are innumerable. One can almost take a map and indicate where new species of Epimys are to be found, namely, on those islands still’ unexplored. Years ago, when the writer was actively engaged in studying the British Mollusca and Lepi- doptera, the question of endemic forms was constantly in mind; but in those days we failed to discriminate properly between the different classes of ‘‘varieties.’? We made the mistake of looking for well-marked “sports” or aberrations, rather than for con- stant but only slightly distinguished local races. There was a practical reason for this, in the fact that by searching the litera- ture we could ascertain whether a well-marked variation had been reported from the continent; whereas the determination of subspecifie types analogous to those described by Miller among 180 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII mammals required long series from different parts of Europe, and these we did not possess, and could not readily obtain, Miller, following the custom of mammalogists, lays great stress on sub- species, but almost ignores individual variations, except such as are expressed by the statistical data regarding size. By reading the synonymy, one can see that many such variations have re- ceived names, and I can not doubt that the time will come when these names will be generally used. In this case, it will be extremely desirable to use the same adjectival name for analogous varieties of different species, and beyond the limits of subspecies it ought not to be held that a name once used in a genus can not be employed again. It may be true that most or all of the ‘‘indi- vidual’’ varieties can be expressed by zygotic formule, but one can not remember all these formule, nor use them in speech with any comfort. Moreover, they have to do with the germinal con- stitution rather than the patent characters. Little provides all his varieties with polynomial English appellations, but would not Latin varietal names be better? Following his theory con- cerning the pigments, some of the varieties receive names which do not suggest the animals at all; thus ‘‘brown-eyed yellow,’’ according to the apparently excellent colored plate, is light orange-ferruginous, while ‘‘sooty-yellow’’ is dark gray with yellowish under parts. Morgan? describes a wild variety of M. musculus from Colorado, which he calls ‘‘mauve,’’ but from the detailed account it is rather ‘‘fauve,’’ namely, fulvous or yellowish brown. It must be similar to the Old World subspecies azoricus, or possibly that subspecies introduced? If we had standard scientific names for the different forms, we should try to compare our specimens with the types or descriptions of those names, and it would not be left to authors to use such miscellane- ous descriptive terms as might occur to them. For Mus musculus, possibly Little’s apparently excellent colored plates might be made the standards for a series of names. Thus his Fig. 9 (pl. 3) is the animal named niger as long ago as 1801; Fig. 10, the dilute black, would naturally take the name nigrescens. Fig. 12 is probably albicans of Billberg, 1827. MAMMALS PECULIAR TO THE BRITISH ISLANDS Insectivora Sorex araneus castaneus (Jenyns 1838). Great Britain. Not so dark as true ANEUS. 8 Ann. N. Y. Acad. Sci., XXI, p. 106. No. 567] SHORTER ARTICLES AND CORRESPONDENCE 181 Sorex granti (Barrett-Hamilton and Hinton 1913). Inner Hebrides. Dif- “7 from araneus by the contrast between bright-colored flanks and upper parts; teeth also different. N ick fodiens bicolor (Shaw 1791). Great Britain. Under parts usually washed with wood-brown instead of buffy whitish; skull smaller. Chiroptera Rhinolophus ferrwm-equinum insulanus Barrett-Hamilton 1910. Central and . Engl Wing shorter. Rhinolophus hipposideros minutus (Montagu 1808). England and Ireland. Wing shorter. Carnivora Mustela erminea stabilis (Barrett-Hamilton 1904). Mainland of Great Britain, er large, with large teeth; color somewhat different, little darker above. Change to white in winter less complete and asl n in continental forms. Mustela erminea ricine (Miller 1907). Islands of Islay and Jura, Scot- land. Smaller than stabilis; proportions of skull different. Mustela hibernica (Thomas and Barrett-Hamilton 1895). Ireland and Isle of Man. Quite distinct; recognized by combination of black-tipped, heavily penciled tail with entirely dark ear and upper lip. Superficially like certain North American forms. Felis sylvestris grampia (Miller 1907). Scotland; formerly throughout Great Britain. Darker, with more pronounced black markings. Rodentia Lepus europeus occidentalis de Winton 1898. England, Scotland and Isle Lepus timidus scoticus (Hilzheimer 1906). Highlands of Scotland. s Lepus hibernicus Bell 1837. Ireland. Distinguished by the strongly russet aae ra partial or complete absence of white winter coat. Scot Rootomye dini Barrett-Hamilton and Hinton 1913. Island of Mull, Hebrides Evotomys glareolus t britannicus (Miller 1900). Great Britain. Smaller; color less inten Evotomys Shiimmenkals Barrett-Hamilton 1903. Skomer Island, off coast of Wales. Color above unusually light and bright; skull peculiar. Microtus agrestis exsul Miller 1908. North and South Uist, Hebrides. Re- sembles true agrestis of Scandinavia; teeth peculiar, a character usually present which elsewhere in the species occurs as a rather rare anomaly. Microtus agrestis macgillivrait Barrett-Hamilton and Hinton 1913. Island of Islay, Hebrides. Microtus agrestis hirtus (Bellamy 1839). England and South Scotland. Smaller than typical agrestis; upper pe noticeably tinged with rus- Set, and venter washed with wood-bro Microtus agrestis neglectus (Jenyns oes Highlands of Scotland. Not So small as hirtus; upper parts darker. 182 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII Microtus orcadensis Millais 1904. South Orkney Islands. Related to M. sarnius of Guernsey and the Pleistocene M. corneri of South England. Distinguished by its large pn and dark color. Microtus — (Millais 1905). Sanday Island, N. Orkney group. Alli o orcadensis, but skull differing; upper parts much lighter. Microtus Pree westre Miller 1908. Westray Island, N. Orkney group. Not so pale as in typical form; teeth differing a little. Arvicola re (L. 1758). Typical Pagans England and South Scotla Large; color moderately dar Arvicola einiiibie ater (Macgillivray TEN = reta Miller 1910. Scot- land, except southward. Darker, melanism frequent. The name was changed on account of Hypudæus terrestris var. ater Billberg 1827, but the change is perhaps needless, as Billberg’s animal was not a sub- Tae and has not been treated as a species or subspecies under pines ayer (de Winton 1895). Lewis and Barra islands, Hebri- des. Large, with small ears; color dark. Apodemus hirtensis (Barrett-Hamilton 1899). a of St. Kilda, Near hebridensis, but skull larger and color dar Apodemus fridariensis (Kinnear 1906). Fair isle, Shetland group. Large; skull peculiar; colors also somewhat peculia p reaR flavicollis wintoni (Barrett- Hamilton 1900), England. Under parts with duller color, pectoral spot more diffus Mus muralis Barrett-Hamilton 1899. Island of St. Kilda. Like M. musculus but feet and tail less slender; skull peculiar. Mus feroensis (Clarke 1904). Faroe Islands. Larger than musculus and muralis; hind foot very robust; tail ape ed. Sciurus vulgaris leucourus Kerr 1792. eat Britain and Ireland. Small; tail drab, fading in summer to cream "buf. No dark phas Ungulata Cervus elaphus scoticus Lönnberg 1906. Great Britain. Color darker and less gray than in the related Norwegian form Capreolus ko thotti Lönnberg. 1916. Great Britain. Darker, face darker than body. I thought it of interest to compare the above British list with a similar one for the Spanish peninsula (Spain and Portugal). The latter area is continuous northward with France, but the Pyrenees constitute a barrier. The Iberian peninsula differs so much in its recent geological history from Britain, and is at the same time so much more southern, that we should expect to find the faunal elements very different. This expectation is realized, yet the difference in numbers between the two lists is not very great, and the number of Iberian forms treated as distinct species is exactly the same (12) as that for the British Islands. This suprising result is evidently due to the numerous sm No. 567] SHORTER ARTICLES AND CORRESPONDENCE 183 islands of the British group, such islands being wanting around the coasts of Spain. MAMMALS PECULIAR TO THE SPANISH (IBERIAN) PENINSULA Ins Talpa cst "(Gabe Galem pyrenaicus pa putes Granth j. Sorex araneus granarius Miller. Neomys anomalus Cabr. Crocidura mimula cantabra Sensi Crocidura russula cintre Mille Erinaceus europeus adinera B.- am Chiroptera rianensis (Graells.). Martes biog mediterranea (Also Balearic Is Mustela putorius aureolus (B.-Ham.). Mungos ose! i (Gray). Genetta genetta (L.), typical subsp. Felis akak tartessia (Miller). Lynx pardellus Miller. (B.-Ham.). Mite nivalis iberica (B.-Ham.). Rodentia Lepus granatensis Rosenb. (Also Balearic Is.). Lepus granatensis gallæcius Miler. UNIVERSITY OF COLORADO Eliomys lusitanicus Memen Glis glis pyrenaicus C Microtus piisa rogianus ke cd Microtus asturia Arvicola sa thes Miller, typical subsp. Pitymys lusitanicus (Gerbe). Pitymys marie (Major). Pitymys pelandonius Miller. Pitymys depressus Miller. Pitymys ibericus (Gerbe), typical subsp. Pitymys ibericus centralis Miller. Mus spicilegus hispanicus Miller. Sciurus vulgaris numantius Miller. Sciurus vulgaris infuscatus (Cabr.). Sciurus vulgaris segure Miller. Sciurus vulgaris beticus (Cabr.). Ungulata Sus scrofa castilianus Thomas, Sus scrofa beticus Thomas. Cervus elaphus hispanicus a Capreolus capreolus can Capra pyrenaica lusitanica (Paina): apra pyrenaica victorie Cabr. Capra pyrenaica hispanica (Schimp.). Rupicapra parva (Cabr.). T. D. A. COCKERELL LITERATURE CITED Bateson, W. Mendel’s Principles of Heredity Cambridge (England) tdia rsity Press. 1909. Castle, W. E. 396 pp. Heredity of Coat papet in Guinea-pigs and Rabbits. Publ. Carnegie Inst. of Wash. No. 2 T L. La loi de Mendel et M a la EEE chez les ouris. 4me note. Darhishire, A, D. Arch, Zool. exp. et gén. Not Notes on the Results of Crossing gk Waltzing Mice with European Albino Races. et Revue, 1905. Biometrika, Vol. 2, p. 101, 1902. 184 ' THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII Doncaster, L. On the psa of Coat Colour in Rats. Proc. Camb. Phil. Soc., Vol. 12, pt. 4, p. 215, 1905. Durham, F. M. A Pilimi Account of s Inheritance of Coat Colors in Mice. Rept. Evol. C’t’ee. Roy. Soc., IV, 1908. Hagedoorn, re L. The Genetic Factors in ae Devan of the House- - mouse which Influence the Coat Color, with Notes on Such Factors in the aie of Other Rodents. Zeit. fiir indukt. Abst. u. Vererb., Bd. 6, pp. 97-136, 1912. poi E and Castle, W. E. Selection and Crossbreeding in Relation to Tikin of Coat-pigments and Coat-patterns in Rats and Mice. rad Carnegie Inst. of Was 1907. Morgan, T. H. Recent Wicpetinante on he Inheritance of Coat Color in Mice. Am. Nart., Vol. 43, pp. 494-510, 1909. NOTES AND LITERATURE SWINGLE! ON VARIATION IN F, CITRUS HYBRIDS AND THE THEORY OF ZYGOTAXIS SWINGLE in two recent papers has published some very inter- esting observations on Citrus species and their F, hybrids. On the basis of these observations, the somewhat startling statement is made that current theories of heredity and variation give no adequate explanation of variability in F, hybrid generations from ‘‘pure bred” parent strains. Swingle assumes this vari- ability to be so great that qualitative differences in chromosomes can not account for it. As the chromosomes in the F, hybrid remain unfused until synapsis, there is said to be no opportunity for quantitative exchange of hereditary substance, so that this variation can not be accounted for on this basis. Hence, if proof can be given to show that in certain specific cases, pairs of gametes of identical hereditary composition? give rise to very diverse organisms, the way has been opened for a general reinvestigation of the validity of our modern theories of heredity. The term ‘‘pure bred’’ as used by Swingle implies that cer- tain Citrus species reproduce themselves in a relatively faithful manner from seed, there being no overlapping of distinguishing Specific characters and very little variation of these characters intraspecifically. C. aurantium and C. trifoliata are examples of such widely separated species. The former has been grown from seed in Florida for two hundred years, and though varia- tions have appeared, they are said to differ but little from the general type of C. aurantium, and in no way to approximate that of C. trifoliata. On the basis of evidence of this kind, Swingle believes the various Citrus species (C. aurantium, C. trifoliata, C. medica limonum, ete.) breed true in nearly all their characters and especially in those which differentiate them from one another. Hence, for genetic studies, the germ cells of these species are t Swingle, W, T., ‘Variation in First Generation Hybrids (Imperfect minance): Its Possible Explanation through Zygotaxis,’’? IV° Conf. In- ternat. de Genetique, Paris, 1911, pp. 381-394; ‘‘Some New Citrus Fruits,” Amer, Breed. Mag., 4: 83-95, 1913. ? The italics are my own. 185 186 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vor. XLVIII assumed, in respect to these differential characters, to be pure; or, expressed in more technical language, each species is for the characters under observation, genotypically homozygous. This assumption is based on wholly inadequate evidence, as will be shown later. Citrus trifoliata crossed with other Citrus species (C. auran- tium, ete.) gave F, hybrid families showing a large degree of variability, even when the seeds from a single cross having identical male and female parents were grown. This variability expressed itself in foliage, habit of growth, and fruit, and was especially noticeable in the latter, the fruits of the F, individuals showing differences in color, size, texture, shape, number of seeds, and flavor. For example, from a single cross of C. trifoliata X C. aurantium, the 11 resulting hybrid seeds gave rise to F, plants (citranges) differing in foliage, habit of growth, and very strik- ingly in fruit. The fruit of one of these citranges, the ‘‘ Morton,”’ was smooth, round, very large, and orange-colored; those of the **Colman’’ were rather flattened, globose, pubescent, yellow, al- most seedless, and lacked the disagreeable oil common to the others; while those of still another type, the ‘‘ Willits,” were often monstrously fingered. The ‘‘Phelps’’ was bitter, while the **Saunders’’ almost lacked this quality. The ‘‘Rustic’’ often has double fruits with many seeds, and a habit of growth more like its aurantium parent. When varieties of the lemon were crossed with C. trifoliata, still greater differences in the F, generation (citremons) resulted. These consisted largely of ‘‘abnormal’’ foliage developments. Hypophylls, though absent in the common Citrus species are ex- tremely characteristic of C. trifoliata. About 20 per cent. of the lemon-trifoliata hybrids developed an intensified form of this character, and this proportion occurred in each case in crosses involving three different varieties of lemon. The tangerine orange X grape fruit (tangelo) in the F, generation was almost as variable as the citrange families. F, hybrids between the West Indian lime and the kumquat (limequat) were strikingly different in such characters as aroma, flavor, acidity of pulp and thickness of skin. Although much stress has been laid on the differences in these F, hybrids, there were numerous similarities. For example, all the Citrus hybrids involving C. trifoliata in their parentage have compound, semi-evergreen leaves, increased hardiness and fruits No. 567] SHORTER ARTICLES AND CORRESPONDENCE 187 with abundant bitterish, acid. juice: Two of the citranges (Cal man and Cunningham) have the pubescent fruit character of C. trifoliata, while the others are smooth-skinned. The author’s data led him to formulate in substance the follow- ing conclusions, which I have grouped and stated in my own language. 1. Citrus species are but slightly variable in the characters which differentiate them, and, in the sense that no overlapping takes place, may be said to breed true, their germ cells being genetically pure for these differential characters. 2. Individual plants of the F, hybrid generations between these species are strikingly variable, although all are, in a given cross, the zygotic product of pairs of gametes of ‘‘identical hereditary composition.’’ 3. Modern theories of heredity can not account for this varia- tion. ; These are not the conclusions, however, in which all present- day geneticists would concur. In the first place, few ‘‘modern’”’ geneticists would take Swingle’s view concerning the ‘“‘pure breeding’’ ability of the various Citrus species, nor even of C. aurantium. Webber, in the Encyclopedia of American Horti- culture, notes that 70 varieties of the common sweet orange are grown within our borders, and although a few varieties are fairly constant, the majority of these do not breed true from seed. Practically the same idea has been gained by certain prominent taxonomists of the genus Citrus. De Candolle specifically calls attention to the remarkable variability of the whole group; and Professor Hume of Florida remarks on the same fact in certain Experiment Station publications. As to the variability among the individuals in the special strains used by Swingle in his breed- ing work, no data are given, so that it can not be affirmed that inbred progeny from them would have been duplicates as far as hereditary characters are concerned. Citrus plants naturally cross fertilize, and from this cause alone no dependence can be placed on their ability to produce progeny, which are exact dupli- cates of themselves when inbred; in fact, the inference is that they would not. “Hence, as far as intraspecific constancy of hereditary characters is concerned, Swingle’s statement can not be accepted until more exact information is produced. Swingle says no interspecific gradations occur between these various species, especially C. trifoliata and C. aurantium. Grant- 188 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII ing this, the two species have clearcut differences in leaves (ever- green or deciduous, unifoliolate or compound), in resistance to cold (difference in ability to withstand certain degrees of tem- perature) and in numerous fruit characters (presence or absence of pubescence, quality of juice, quantity of seed, size of fruit, ete.). From the standpoint of modern theories of heredity as regards variation in F, hybrid generations, it matters little whether so- called species intergrade or whether their differences are clear-cut and all variation is intraspecific. In either case, if crosses were made, variation among the F, individuals from a single family might or might not occur. In either case, no violence to modern theories of heredity would result and no new problems would arise. But if two species that differ from each other in part or all of their characters, but breed true intra-specifically (geno- typically homozygous) are crossed, and F, variation results, then modern theories of heredity would be compelled to change front and invoke the aid of new hypotheses. Swingle’s data, assuming that intraspecific variation in Citrus species occurs, does not present a problem of this kind at all. C. aurantium and C. tri- foliata each possess distinctive characters, but convincing data are not at hand to warrant any belief in the homozygosity of these differential characters or of even those the two species may have in common. The evidence directly, and one might almost say conclusively, opposes such a conclusion. If these species are not homozygous in all of their characters, then one can not affirm, in the light of modern theories, that all the gametes produced by a particular group of individuals called a species are identical in hereditary composition, nor even that the gametes of one indi- vidual of such a species are identical as to hereditary potenti- alities. At the risk of wasting valuable space by repeating what is extremely common knowledge to genetice students, let us assume, for the purpose of argument, that C. aurantium and C. trifoliata are homozygous in all their respective characters except one. In the former, the character A is heterozygous and peculiar to this species, Likewise, in C. trifoliata, B is heterozygous and differ- ential. All the remaining characters of the two species may be symbolized, respectively, by the formule XX and YY. When XX Aabb (C. aurantium) is crossed with YYaaBd (C. trifoliata), the resulting progeny would appear in the approximate propor- tion of 1XYAaBb:1XYAabb:1XYaaBb:1XYaabb, providing No, 567] NOTES AND LITERATURE 189 A and B are single factor characters. In the majority of char- acters, the F, hybrids would be intermediate or possess those of either one or the other parent, since all the F, individuals would be alike as far as any hereditary quality symbolized by XY is con- cerned, providing the plants were all grown under the same en- vironmental conditions. But these F, individuals would not be alike as regards the inheritance of the characters A and B. Ex- perimental evidence from crosses of this kind show us that four different F, forms may result, the distinctions between them aris- ing from the presence or absence, through inheritance, of the characters A and B. Dominance is assumed to be absent in this illustration. Swingle’s Citrus hybrids, though involving greater complexity because a large number of parental characters instead of two are probably heterozygous, are of the same general type as those of the illustration and lend themselves to the same interpretation. Owing to the absence of sufficient exact experimental data, one can not speak of unit characters and factors in these hybrids, but one may say without violence to modern theories of heredity that one or both of the parents involved in the crosses which produced the Colman and the Cunningham were heterozygous in the factors or factor for pubescence, that various size factors were hetero- zygous and that one parent was homozygous for absence and one for presence of the factors for hardiness, compound leaves and evergreen foliage. F, variation in Citrus hybrids then, in the light of the data at hand, apparently results from differences in the gametie compo- sition of the heterozygous parents. Swingle calls attention to other cases of variation in F, hy- brids from two pure stocks which support his contention that this phenomenon of F, variation is very general, though usually obscured through variation due to heterozygous parent stock. Collins and Kempton? crossed a race of corn breeding true to waxy endosperm with one constant for horny endosperm. Horny endosperm was dominant in F, and the F, generation segregated ìn the expected ratio of 1 waxy to 3 horny kernels. This ratio represented the average proportion of each when the ears of all the plants were lumped together. The F, progeny of each selfed 8 Collins, G, N., and Kempton, J., II, 1912, ‘‘ Inheritance of Waxy Endo- sperm in Hybrids of Chinese Corn,’’ IV° Conf. Internat. de Genetique, 1911, P. 347; also Cire, No. 120, Bur. of P, I., U. S. Dept. of Agr., 1913. 190 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vow. XLVIII F, plant when taken by itself gave some ears as low as 13.7 per cent. waxy, while others exceeded the expected proportions and gave ears as high as 33.3 per cent. waxy. The investigators point out that this variation is not the result of the laws of chance as the deviation is far greater in many cases than the probable error. Therefore, says Swingle, there can be no doubt but that their varying percentages represented real differences in the hereditary composition of the first generation plants. It would be hard to find a more conclusive case since there could be no doubt as to the purity of the parents and what is more rare no possible doubt as to whether a given kernel had a waxy or a horny endosperm. Mendelians are said to be unaware how fatal this phenomena is to some of the chief tenets of modern theories of heredity, and they are also accused, somewhat unjustly, I believe, of applying the term ‘‘imperfect dominance’’ to this and to the Citrus phenomena. : In this case, both parents were undoubtedly homozygous for their respective endosperm characters, so that heterozygosity will not account satisfactorily for the deviations. But this is a dif- ferent phenomena than Swingle found in his Citrus hybrids, for here one is dealing with a fluctuation in a proportion or ratio involving the same character, while in his experiments the diffi- culty was the variation in presence and absence of distinct and often new characters, indicating an extremely heterozygous parentage. As an explanation or working hypothesis for his own and similar data, Swingle advances a somewhat new and suggestive chromosome theory on the assumption that it fills an urgent need. The theory of zygotaxis, as it is called, may be summarized as follows: Maternal and paternal chromosomes probably persist side by side in the cells, unchanged in quality and number throughout the whole development of the F, organism. This being true, Swingle, in order to explain his data, assumes that the influence in character formation exerted by chromosomes on the F, hybrids, is in some cases due to their relative positions in the nucleus, and that these relative positions result from accident or at least are determined at the moment of nuclear fusion in fertilization, and remain unchanged in succeeding cell generations. He further No. 567] NOTES AND LITERATURE 191 assumes that those chromosomes lying nearest the nuclear wall (peripheral) are better nourished than those centrally located, and hence they exert more influence in character formation, and dominating synapsis, produce gametes similar in their hereditary character to the cells of the first generation hybrids, whose char- acter in turn was determined at fertilization by the configuration the chromosomes took in the fusion nucleus. On this theory, reversions, sports, etc., may result from sudden changes in the nuclear configuration. Three types of nuclear configuration are assumed to occur in higher organisms, the character and effects of which are synop- tically outlined below. 1. Interspecific Hybrids—Usually sterile and intermediate. Chromosomes repel each other and occupy opposite sides of the F, zygote nuclei, exerting equal influence in the ontogeny of F, organisms, explaining why first generation hybrids of this char- acter are always intermediate, little variable and usually sterile. Synapsis often impossible. 2. Mendelian Crosses—Abnormally inbred races of domesti- cated animals and plants. F, generation usually intermediate, fertile, dialytic at synapsis. Dominance of certain characters in these hybrids is due to the inherited potentialities of the chromo- somes rather than to their nuclear positions. 3. Normal Cross-bred Species—Probably normal in wild Species. Hybrids usually vigorous, fertile, and variable. Free intermingling of chromosomes in the fusion nucleus at fertiliza- tion. Nuclear configuration permanent for each individual. Synapsis normal. This elaborate and attractive theory, based admittedly to a great degree on assumptions, is advanced by Swingle in the belief that it will help to clarify the problems of heredity, even though he acknowledges it does not help one to arrive at satisfactory explanations. In the reviewer’s opinion, however, the field of genetics is already burdened with enough theories of this par- ticular type and the somewhat unnecessary but ever-increasing new additions serve to confuse rather than clarify the ideas of the average student of genetics. Besides, Swingle’s assumption that maternal and paternal chromosomes in the cells of F, hybrids repel each other and do not mingle in the F, zygote cells is not borne out by the few cytological facts at our command. Rosen- 192 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII berg’s* work on species hybrids of Drosera, Moenkhaus’s* investi- gations of species hybrids in fish and some work on certain hybrids in the Echinodermata group give us facts that directly oppose such an assumption. As a further criticism, one may say that most biologists who have had experience with pedigree cul- tures would decidedly criticize the synoptic outline and the nar- row sphere assigned to Mendelian phenomena. Aside from the theoretical considerations, these two papers con- tain descriptions of Citrus-like species new to occidental horti- culture, together with a somewhat detailed account of the various Citrus hybrids and their hardiness and practical value, showing the truly fine results achieved by the workers in this field toward moving the Citrus belt northward and adding new varieties of this genus to the world’s horticulture. ORLAND E. WHITE BROOKLYN BOTANIC GARDEN December 4, 8 Rosenberg, O., ‘‘Cytologische und Morphologische Studien an Drosera longifolia X D. rotundifolia,’’ Kungl. Svenska Vetenskapsakademiens Hand- linger., 43, N: ou, pp. 1-64, 1909. 4 Tafn. 4 Moenkhaus, W. J., ‘‘The Development of the Hybrids between Fundulus heteroclitus and Menidia notata with especial reference to the Behavior of the Maternal and Paternal Chromatin,’’ Amer. Jour. of Anatomy, 3; 29-65, 1904. Plates I-IV. VOL. XLVIII, NO. 568,“ APRIL, 1914 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST A MONTHLY JOURNAL Devoted to the Advancement of the Biological Sciences with Special Reference to the Factors of Evolution CONTENTS Page I. The Origin of X Capsella Bursa-pastoris arachnoidea. Dr. HENRI Hus - 193 - - 236 Dr. A. FRANKLIN SHULL H Biology of the Thysanoptera. II. II. Shorter Articles and Discussion: Barriers as to Distribution as regards Birds and Mammals. JOSEPH GRINNELL. Yellow Varieties of Rats. Professor W. E. CASTLE soo ooo oo a G ee a a a IV. Notes and Literature: Woods on Heredity and the “ Influence of Monarchs.” M LK a8 wi i 8 ž es Sas = = a = - - 255 THE SCIENCE PRESS LANCASTER, PA. GARRISON, N. Y. NEW YORK: SUB-STATION 84 The American Naturalist S. intended for publication and books, etc., intended for review Cares be MS sent to the Editer of THE AMERICAN NATURALIST, Garrison- -on-Hud yter y Short maries of articles containing sum w York. research work bear gp the problems of organic evolution are peneers welcome, and will be given pe Paors in pais atio rea reprints of spider are supplied to authors free of charge. e hundr Further reprints will be supplied at cost riptions and advertisements should be sent to the gma The or anadian postage twenty-five cents additional. postage is fifty c and eign ents The cha mee for single aust is forty cents. The advertising rates are Four Dollars for a pa THE SCIENCE PRESS NEW YORK: Sub-Station 84 Lancaster, Pa. Entered as second-class matter, April 2, 1908, at the Garrison, N. Y. Post Office at Lancaster, Pa., under the Act of Congress of March 3, 1879 FOR SALE ARCTIC, eee nine GREENLAND DS’ S Well Herth ‘eo Prices Particulars of ESEN, Bird Collector Husavik, North Iceland, Via Leidle, England WANTED TO PURCHASE a set of BIRDS OF AMERICA by J. J. Audubon, 7 or 8 volumes, please report, stating cash price, stat- ing condition, binding and dates of volumes. F. C. HARRIS, Box 2244 Boston, Massachusetts TO OOLOGISTS and ENTOMOLOGISTS W. F. H. ROSENBERG Importer of Exotic Zoological Specimens 57, Haverstock Hill, London, N. W., England Begs to announce the publication of a new Price List (No. 20) of et Sg — k over 400 species from various parts of the world. These will be mailed free on application, as well as any of the following lists: Birdskins (over 5000 species); Birds’ Eggs (over 1,100 species) ; Reptiles, Batrachians and Fishes (over Sg species) ; Exotic Lepidoptera (over 8,000 species Largest stock in the world of specimens in all branches of Zoology. All Museums and Amateurs should write for these Lists All specimens sent on approval. Please state which lists are required and give name of this periodical. Photographic Films Developed 5 cents per Spool, Prints 3 cents up, Post Cards 5 cents. 8x10 Enlargements 25 cents mounted. Send for Samples and Price List. FRANK R. BARBEAU y 89 West Bridge Street Oswego, N. Y. The Sge of Chicago Arts, Literature, Commerce and phe # Law, Medicine, Educa- . Instructs embers of the 4 tion, Summer enas, 1914 15-July 22 oo Tem July 23-Aug. 28 announcements will be sent upon application. The University of Chicago Illinois (L Mitchell Tower Chicago Lo n THE AMERICAN NATURALIST Vor. XLVIII April, 1914 No. 568 THE ORIGIN OF x CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ` ARACHNOIDEA DR. HENRI HUS UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN Sivce Jordan! described a number of elementary spe- cies of Capsella Bursa-pastoris, their constancy has been a subject of cultural experiment. Herbarium material demonstrates the existence of numerous apparently unde- scribed forms. The finding of strikingly distinct forms, such as Capsella Heegeri? and, more recently, C. Viguieri,? the work of Almquist‘ and that of Shull have added to the interest which this species holds for the investigator. It was Shull who determined the zygotic constitution of various forms. To be able to demonstrate this with ex- actitude is of the greatest value since Bateson and Lotsy expressed their doubt as to the homozygocity of de Vries’s nothera Lamarckiana. It was left to Nilsson® to clearly Show its necessarily heterozygous character. The inter- est aroused by this paper® leads me to believe that an 1 Jordan, A., ‘ Diagnoses d’espéces nouvelles ou méconnues pour servir de matériaux à une flore réformée de la France et des contrées voisines.’’ Paris, 1864. _*Solms-Laubach, H. Graf zu, ‘‘Cruciferen studien. I. Capsella heegeri, eme neuentstandene Form der deutschen Flora,’’ Bot. Zeit., 55: 167, pl. 7, 1900, *Blaringhem, L., ‘‘Les transformations brusques des êtres vivants.’’ Paris, 1911. *Almquist, E., ‘Studien über die Capsella Bursa-pastoris (L.),’’ Acta Horti Bergiani, 4: No. 6, 1907. 5 Heribert-Nilsson, N., ‘Die Variabilität der @inothera Lamarckiana und das Problem der Mutation,’’ Zeitschr. f. ind. Abst. u. Vererb., 8: 89, 1912. * Lotsy, J. P., ‘‘ Fortschritte unserer Anschauungen über Deszendenz seit Darwin und der jetzige Standpunkt der Frage,’’ Progressus Rei Botanicæ, 4: 361, 1913. 193 194 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII account of certain cultures of Capsella, in which muta- tions were simulated, would be of timely interest. During the winter of 1908-1909, I collected in a green- house at Ann Arbor, Michigan, and at the disposal of the Botanical Department of the University of Michigan, twelve rosets of Capsella Bursa-pastoris, the leaves of which showed certain more or less striking morphological differences. With the hope of isolating certain biotypes, the rosets were placed in pots and permitted to flower. No measures were taken to prevent the accidental trans- ference of pollen, but the pots were placed about six inches apart. This, as will be shown later, is the only precaution necessary to guard against cross-pollination, provided the cultures are carried on in a greenhouse and during the winter months. After a portion of the seed had ripened, the plants, the majority of which retained their climax leaves, became herbarium specimens. More recently, after constant association has enabled me to detect minute differences, it has been possible to identify some of these plants with two of the biotypes described by Shull,” to wit, rhomboidea and simplex. At the time of collection, the differences were sensed, but could not be described technically, since the extent of the influence wielded by fluctuating variability was an unknown quan- tity. Never before had I so fully realized the truth of de Vries’s statement.’ We are trained to the appreciation of the differentiating marks of systematic species. ... Our minds are turned from the delicately shaded features which differentiate elementary species. The seed obtained was sown in sterilized soil during the spring of 1910. From each seedpan 60 individuals were transplanted to flats. As the plants grew older, it was found that, with a single exception, the seedlings in each of the flats were uniform, but that the seedlings in the different flats were not alike, three types being dis- tinguishable. The interest in these types, for the isola- 7 Shull, G. H., ‘‘ Bursa bursa-pastoris and Bursa Heegeri: Biotypes and Hybrids,’’ Publ. No. 112, Carnegie Institution of Washington, 1909. 8 de Vries, Hugo, ‘‘Species and Varieties,’’ 689, 1905. No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 195 tion of which these cultures had been undertaken, soon yas overshadowed by the behavior of the seedlings bear- ing the number 4,108.6 and which were the offspring yielded by a plant of a type not described by Shull and which I have named X Capsella Bursa-pastoris Setchelli- ana, in honor of Professor William Albert Setchell. FIG. 1. APPEARANCE OF A LINEAR-LEAVED FORM AMONG SEEDLINGS OF Capsella Bursa-pastoris. During the time that the seedlings remained in the seed- pan, no deviations from the expected course of develop- ment were noted. However, after the seedlings had been transplanted to flats and had remained there a week or So, it became evident that some of the seedlings were not making the expected growth. Their development ap- peared most insignificant compared with that of the majority. A closer examination showed the cotyledons to be somewhat larger than normal and the leaves proper to be exceedingly small and almost linear. Nor did they attain the same length as the leaves of the rosets belong- ing to other types. 196 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII An explanation of this peculiar development was sought in a possible attack on the part of either fungi or bacteria or in soil conditions. But the latter were uniform for the entire flat. Neither fungi nor bacteria could be demon- strated nor did the underground portion of the ‘‘ab- normal’’ plants look unhealthy or underdeveloped. Fic. 2. SEEDLINGS or æ C.... Setchelli AND « C.... arachnoides At this stage the flat presented the appearance shown in Fig. 1. At the time but three types were distinguished, the first of these constituted by plants which showed an incision of the blade, the second composed of those which apparently had entire leaves, and a third, comprising the small and linear-leaved rosets, which, because of the spider- like appearance of the latter, has been designated X Cap- sella Bursa-pastoris arachnoidea. There also appeared a No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 197 single individual which, while closely resembling the form arachnoidea, differed from it in having somewhat spatu- late leaves. This plant, a plant of arachnoidea and two of Setchelliana, are shown in Fig. 2. After photographs had been taken, the plants were potted and placed in the frames. None of the plants made a growth as vigorous as that of the Capsellas grow- ing in the open. The plants of the form arachnoidea de- veloped leaves with a greatest length of 15 mm. and a greatest width of a little over 1 mm., causing the plant to retain its spider-like appearance. The roset with spatu- late leaves appeared somewhat more vigorous, the aver- age leaf measuring 22 mm. in length, with a greatest width of 2.5mm. In later generations I have been able to ob- tain rosets of arachnoidea with a greatest leaf-length of 100 mm. and a greatest width of 6 mm. In the frames, flowering shoots made their appearance, those on arachnoidea being remarkable chiefly because of their small size, reaching a length not exceeding 12 em. The flowers were small but well-formed. No well-devel- oped pollen could be demonstrated. Seed did not form and the capsules retained their original form, typical of non-fertile capsules in Capsella Bursa-pastoris, remind- ing one of the capsules of Capsella Heegeri. They do not resemble the fertile capsules of C. procumbens. In the next generation I saw a single capsule formed on arach- noidea as the result of cross-fertilization, and in this case it differed in no manner from the normal capsule such as we know it in Capsella Bursa-pastoris. The ‘‘normal’’ plants, i. e., all those not belonging to the form arachnoidea, matured a large amount of seed. No measures were taken to prevent cross-pollination, but no other plant of Capsella Bursa-pastoris, within a radius of twenty feet, was in flower. At this time, another attempt was made to group the plants. It was found that the criterion used earlier, i. e., the incision of the blade, no longer could be relied upon, Since plants, which at the time of the previous count, had Shown an entire margin, now were more or less incised. 198 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII Unfortunately, after the seed had been collected, the plants were destroyed, having lost their climax leaves. An attempt to group them later withtheaid of photographs failed, be- cause photographs of all plants were taken during the earlier stages only, i. e., before the appearance of the cli- max leaves. Another classification, for which climax leaves are not essential, and which is based upon the relative width of the first six or eight leaves, yields for 54 plants the proportion: ‘‘wide’’ 31, ‘‘narrow’’ 16, ‘‘linear’’ 7, the ideal proportion, as since worked out, being 33:16: 16. The fact that the number for ‘‘linear,’’? which represents the form arachnoidea, is too small by 9, may be ascribed to various circumstances, among others the fact that the last row in the flat did not appear in the photograph upon which the count was based. It is in the last row of a flat one ordinarily meets with the smaller or at least less vig- orous individuals and it is very probable that in this last row occurred a large percentage of individuals belonging to arachnoidea. Furthermore, not all the seedlings, but only sixty, were taken in each case. Almost unconsciously one selects the largest individuals when transplanting from seedpan to flat. It is probable that in this process there were eliminated a greater percentage of seedlings of the linear form than of any of the others. Hence no great weight can be attached to the proportion obtained. The collection of seed brought the work for 1910 to a close. As far as I was aware, no forms similar to arach- noidea had been either noted or described by any one who had devoted his time to culture experiments with Cap- sella. Neither Shull in America, nor Almquist in Swe- den, nor Lotsy® in Holland, has made mention of such forms in their publications. The fact that no seed was produced by the aberrant form seemed to hold out little hope for the continuation of the cultures, and the sole trace left by this new form, if taxonomic form it was, threatened to consist of but a few photographs and some aleohol specimens. A single possibility presented itself. 9 Lotsy, J. P., ‘‘Vorlesungen über Deszendenztheorien,’’ 1: 180, Jena, 1906. No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 199 Whether the parent plant was of a hybrid character or whether the parent plant was mutating, and the new form or forms were to be looked upon as mutants, in either case there existed the possibility, if not the probability, that from the seeds obtained from those plants of the second generation which appeared ‘‘normal,’’ a third gen- eration might be obtained which would again present the abnormal form. Such indeed proved to be the case. 3 Ur Fic. 3. EARLY STAGES IN THE DEVELOPMENT OF BROAD-LEAVED, NARROW-LEAVED AND LINEAR-LEAVED FORMS OF Capsella. The seed for the next generation was obtained from 19 plants. The seed was sown separately in pots of steril- ized soil. Certain of the parent plants, which we now identify with Shull’s simplex and rhomboidea, produced a uniform, broad-leaved offspring. Others behaved like the parent, the form arachnoidea appearing in 197 indi- viduals out of a total of 979, which does not include the 713 which bred true to the broad type. (For an illustra- tion of these types see Fig. 3.) It is unnecessary to go into details as to the various theories which suggested themselves as a solution of the 200 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [VoL. XLVIII origin of the linear-leaved form which, because of its striking appearance, concentrated the attention upon it- self. That perhaps we were dealing with a mutation was a thought which most naturally obtruded itself upon the mind of one who, for years, had fruitlessly tested a large number of species in the hope of discovering a case analogous to that of @nothera Lamarckiana.’® The possibility of a cross between a local form and either Capsella Heegeri or C. procumbens, suggested itself. However, the seedling stage of either of these two forms does not bear the remotest resemblance to that of Cap- sella arachnoidea. At the same time there was slight reason for believing that either Capsella Heegeri or Cap- sella procumbens ever had been grown in Ann Arbor. During 1911 and the greater part of 1912, the problem rested here, no satisfactory explanation being found. But pedigree cultures were continued until, on the one hand, we succeeded in placing the plants in optimum sur- roundings for the production of climax leaves, and on the other began to distinguish between the various biotypes. THe BIOTYPES As has been noted previously, it was possible to use two criteria for the classification of the rosets. Leaving out of consideration the rosets of the linear-leaved arach- noidea, it was found that after dividing the rosets accord- ing to the ‘‘broad’’ or ‘‘narrow”’ character of the earlier leaves (Fig. 3), it was possible to further subdivide each group on the basis of the marginal indentation of the leaves subsequently formed. I. The “Broad”? Group.—Here the first four or five leaves possess a blade which is approximately twice as long as broad. Up to this stage the margin remains entire. When the sixth leaf appears one ordinarily can begin to distinguish between two types. These are: Type 1.—In this, the first of the two broad-leaved forms, the margin of the first eight leaves remains entire, 10 Hus, H., ‘‘The Origin of Species in Nature,’’ AMERICAN NATURALIST, 45: 646, Nov., 1911. No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 201 showing at most a very slight crenation (Fig. 4). Usually the ninth leaf, though sometimes it is the eighth and some- times the tenth, shows a more marked indentation, though seldom of a depth of more than 2 mm. on each side of the leaf and slightly below the middle. Subsequent leaves show an increase in the number and depth of the i FIG, 4. DISSECTION OF XOVRG Rosers or C. . . . simpler AND Ọ. . . . rhom- boidea, ‘Suowixe THE ‘*‘ BROAD” Cniniceen OF THE Reena Leaves AND THE DISTINCTIVE CHARACTER OF THE FIRST SIN indentations, the maximum for both being reached in the climax leaves. which usually show five indentations reach- ing about midway from margin to midrib. In those of the earlier leaves which show a marked incision the lobes are obtuse. In the later leaves the lobes become acute. It may be stated as a general truth, that an increase in the depth of the sinus carries with it an increase in sharp- ness of the lobe. There is no secondary lobing, but some- times the margin of the sinus shows a slight denticulation. While in the earlier leaves the sinuses separating the terminal lobe from the rest of the blade are the deepest, 202 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII the converse is true in the later leaves, where the sinuses separating the terminal lobe are the most shallow. I have identified this form with Shull’s simplex! My plants also agree fairly well with the illustration of onto- genetic succession of leaf forms in Bursa ... simplex, shown by Shull.!2 Type 2.—In the second of the two forms distinguished because of the greater relative width of their first leaves, the margin of the first five leaves remains entire, as in the case of those of type 1 (simplex). The sixth leaf, however, ordinarily shows a marked indentation, at least 3 mm. deep and slightly below the middle of the blade (Fig. 4). This indentation may appear in one margin or in both. The lower margin of the sinus ordinarily is at right angles to the midrib, the upper margin making an angle of 45 degrees with the midrib (Fig.7,b). Even when it has become difficult to distinguish between types on the basis of relative width of the earlier roset leaves, italways is possible to distinguish between type 2 (rhomboidea) and type 4 (Setchelliana and Treleaseana), by means of the character of the sinus. In type 4, the lower margin of the sinus makes an angle of 45 degrees with the midrib, while the upper margin makes an angle of between 30 and 45 degrees with the midrib. Hence the first sinus in C.... Setchelliana and C. ... Treleaseana is at least 90 degrees, while the first sinus in rhomboidea measures seldom more than 45 degrees and frequently less. The seventh leaf of plants belonging to type 2 ordi- narily shows two indentations on both sides of the leaf, dividing the blade into a lower portion, two central lobes and a terminal lobe. The depth of the incision amounts to about three-fourths of the width of the blade from mid- rib to margin. It is possible to delay the appearance of the first inden- tations by transplanting from seedpan to flat either too early or too late. In such cases, the indentations appear in the seventh leaf only, or even later, and are rather 11 Loc. cit., 25, and Pl. 2, Fig. 2. 12 Shull, G. H., ‘‘ Verh. d. naturf. Ver. in Briinn,’’ 49, Pl. 4, 1911. No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 203 shallow, reaching a depth of three-fourths of the width of the leaf from margin to midrib in the eighth, ninth or tenth leaf. However, once the indentations have made their appearance, the leaf next produced ordinarily shows two sinuses on both sides of the blade, usually the upper set, rarely the lower, being the deeper of the two, and almost reaching the midrib. The succeeding leaves show an increase in the number of lateral lobes from two to six. Since the incisions almost, if not quite, reach the midrib, both lateral lobes and the terminal lobes are well defined. Upon the lateral lobes secondary lobes appear, ` both on the distal and proximal margins. It is to be noted that only the climax leaves of well-grown specimens of the homozygotic form distinctly show the lobing of the proximal margin and this only on the middle lobes. The lobing of the primary lobes results in the setting off of a small terminal portion of each lateral lobe, which possesses a more or less rhomboidal form. This terminal lobe of the primary lobe can be observed to advantage only in the climax leaves of well-developed specimens. I have no hesitation in identifying type 2 with Shull’s rhomboidea.18 Capsella Bursa-pastoris simplex and C. Bursa-pastoris rhomboidea, described, respectively, as types 1 and 2, agree in having the first five or six leaves twice as long as broad, thus contrasting sharply with the plants to be de- scribed under types 3 and 4, which constitute the ‘‘nar- row’’ group. II. The ‘‘Narrow’’ Group—tIn the plants belonging here, the first five or six leaves possess a blade which is from 2} to 3 times as long as broad. Usually after the appearance of the seventh leaf, sometimes not until the appearance of the tenth leaf, it is possible, on the basis of marginal indentation, to separate the plants with ‘‘nar- row” roset-leaves into two groups, designated respec- tively types 3 and 4. Type 3.—Rosets of plants belonging to type 3 can not be distinguished from those of type 4, until after the 78 Shull, Verh., Pl. 2; Biotypes, Pl. 1, Fig. 2. 204 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII seventh leaf has appeared (Fig. 5). Itis to be noted that for the first six leaves of type 4, the ratio between mean length and width is 6:2, while for the corresponding leaves of type 3, the same ratio is 5:2. Once the seventh -< — — se th x Fic. 5. DISSECTION oF YOUNG RosETS OF @ C. .. . Setchelli AND g O. ... ep pet SHOWING THE “ NARROW ” CHARACTER OF THE EARLIER ES AND THE DISTINCTIVE CHARACTER OF THE FIRST SINUS. leaf has appeared, a distinction readily can be made, since in type 3, no sinuses appear, and the leaves, from the seventh to the tenth, might be mistaken for those of simplex (Fig. 5). Later leaves readily can be distin- guished from those of simplex, by the pointed apex, the very shallow sinuses, ending in a sharp tooth, and by the fact that the greatest width of the blade lies above the middle, about one third the length from the tip (Fig. 6). This form, which because of its morphological charac- ters on the one hand, and its behavior in breeding on the other, can readily be distinguished from all others, I designate X Capsella Bursa-pastoris attenuata. Type 4.—Not only do the first leaves of plants, belong- ing to this type, differ in relative width from the first leaves of plants of rhomboidea and simplex, but there also No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 205 is a difference in the apex of the leaf, the apices of leaves of this type, like those of type 3, being decidedly pointed, while those of types 1 and 2 are rounded." At the sixth or seventh leaf stage, the marginal indentations make their appearance, at first as slight crenations, then as long and shallow sinuses, and finally, in the eighth or ninth leaf, as a sinus on one or both sides of the midrib and about the middle of the blade (Fig. 5). The lower margin of the first sinus ordinarily makes an angle of 45 degrees with the midrib, while the upper margin makes an angle of from 30 to 45 degrees with the midrib. This renders the first sinus ordinarily greater than 90 degrees (Fig. 7, a). The depth of the first sinus is ap- Had con eel tee ee proximately one half the... . attenuata axv © i distance from margin to midrib. In subsequent leaves the depth increases, so that in the 11th leaf the sinuses almost reach the midrib. In T'releasi, one of the two forms, which together constitute type 4, the climax leaves show incisions to the midrib, and a well-marked terminal lobe, while in the other the sinuses are less deep but the terminal lobe still is well marked (Fig. 8). The number of sinuses increases in propor- 14 It is to be noted that in my cultures there appear, from time to time, plants of rhomboidea of which the leaves have sharply pointed lobes. What relation these plants bear to others classed with them under rhomboidea, I am at present unable to say. 206 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. XLVIII tion to their depth. If the seventh leaf has one sinus in each margin, the eighth and ninth usually have two, the tenth and eleventh, three, and so on, until the mean of six is reached. As the lobes increase in number, they Fic. T. EARLY ROSET LEAVES OF @ Fig. 8. CLIMAX meag OF & . «s+ Setehelt ARD C. .; rhom- ares patchetii's AND & ory & & dh boidea. not only become narrower but the sinuses do likewise. This is the result of a gradual increase in the angle between the lower margin of the sinus and the midrib. In the eighth leaf the lower margin forms an angle of about 90 degrees with the midrib, causing the formation of a primary lobe, triangular in shape and with an upper angle of about 45 degrees, instead of the 90-degree angle found in the first lobe. In older leaves the angle between lower margin of sinus and midrib may increase to 110 or even 120 degrees. The climax leaves therefore get to resemble more and more those of rhomboidea, especially since the distal margin of the sinus, from the tenth leaf on, exhibits a number of denticulations which, in older leaves, especially of one of the forms (Treleaseana), tend to become incisions, so that secondary lobes are No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 207 formed. However, the end of the lobes of early leaves of type 4 always are sharply pointed (Fig. 9), while the lobes of early leaves of rhomboidea are ordi- narily rounded at the ends (Fig. 4). RGH {iei FIG. ¢ hasidin ae © UPPER Row: 3 SETS or LEAVES FROM AS MANY PLANTS OF @ C.... Treleasi. Lowrer Row: 4 SETS OF LEAVES FROM AS MANY LANTS OF g C. Setchelii. From a morphological point of view these leaves are entirely different from any form described by Shull, the differences being most marked and very readily recog- nized once our attention has been called tothem. But it is especially the behavior of the plants on breeding which leads me to recognize them as most distinct hybrid forms and which,I have designated X Capsella Bursa-pastoris Setchelliana in honor of Professor William Albert Setchell, and X Capsella Bursa-pastoris Treleaseana, in honor of Professor William Trelease. Type 5—Capsella Bursa-pastoris arachnoidea. This form, which readily is recognized from the first by its linear leaves, does not require an elaborate description at present, since it will be discussed in detail later. It has been illustrated in Figs. 1, 2 and 3. 208 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST — [Vou. XLVIII The above descriptions apply only to plants grown under fairly uniform conditions, in a light soil in a green- house, and treated in such a manner as to offer the plant the most favorable conditions for development. By leav- ing the plants too long in the flats, so that crowding re- sults, by keeping them too moist and warm, etc., it is possible to produce abnormal climax leaves in which the typical differences can be recognized with difficulty only. By leaving plants too long in the seedpans, by keeping them too dry, it may be brought about that plants flower without having produced climax leaves. There will be doubtless many who, because of this, will refuse recogni- tion to the segregates just described. ‘‘Quacunque dixi, si placuerint, dictavit auditor.” Fortunately, the differ- ences of behavior on breeding are such, we must recognize their distinct genotypic constitution. GENOTYPIC CONSTITUTIONS _ Shull, in the papers above quoted, made one of the most important of recent contributions to science, since he de- termined with exactitude the relations existing between some of the lesser forms which, because of their alleged constancy or inconstancy, have been a bone of contention since the days of Jacquin. Making extensive cultures of Capsella, Shull was able to distinguish four forms (Fig. 10), to wit, heteris, with leaves divided to the midrib, with J Fic. 10. CLIMAX LEAves OF C. .. . heteris, C. . . . tenuis, C. . . . rhomboidea AND 0. . , , simplex. No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 209 elongated primary lobes, a marked secondary lobe, in the distal axil of the primary lobe and a well-marked terminal lobe; rhomboidea, with leaves divided to the midrib, with an unelongated primary lobe, with an incision in the distal margin setting off a secondary lobe and a corresponding incision on the proximal margin of the primary lobe, set- ting off, in well-grown specimens, a terminal portion of each lateral lobe, generally of rhomboidal form; tenuis, with the elongated primary lobe of heteris, but with a sinus which usually does not reach the midrib, terminal lobe clear cut; simplex, with lateral lobes obtuse, never attenuated, the incisions being shallow and never reach- ing the midrib. Shull recognized here the presence and absence of two factors, one (A) responsible for the sharp primary lobe of heteris and the attenuation of the lobes in tenuis, while the other (B) is responsible for the division of the leaf to the midrib, the definite terminal lobe and the second- ary lobes. On this basis Shull was able to represent the biotypes by conventional Mendelian symbols, thus: heteris, AB; rhomboidea, aB; tenuis, Ab; simplex, ab. That this conventional presentation gives us a reliable working basis, my experiments have shown most satis- factorily. With the aid of these symbols I have been able to solve the origin of Capsella arachnoidea, the experi- ments showing that, without question, forms presenting the spider-like appearance of the rosets typical of this plant are of hybrid origin. THE Zycotic Constirution oF 4,108.6 The problem to be solved was that of the zygotic con- stitution of the original parent, the plant which in my notes is recorded as 4,108.6. Among its offspring neither heteris nor tenuis made their appearance, while both rhomboidea (aB) and simplex (ab) were met with. Hence the parent was homozygotie for (a), but heterozygotic for (B). Therefore, its zygotic constitution, in part, must have been aaBb. Besides rhomboidea and simplex there appeared two 210 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII forms, referred to as types 3 and 4, the latter being ca- pable of further subdivision. Neither of these was de- scribed by Shull. At least one difference between rhom- boidea and simplex, on the one hand, and types 3 and 4, on the other, could be noted at once, i. e., the relative width of the leaf. As has been shown above, the former have their first leaves twice as long as broad, the latter three times as long as broad. The idea suggested itself that there might exist a factor which determined these charac- ters. Since the original parent belonged to type 4, the narrow character of the earlier leaves must be dominant over the broad character. Also, since the original parent produced both ‘‘narrow’’ and ‘‘broad’’ types, it must have been heterozygotic for this character. Using (N) to indicate the gene, we get for the zygotic construction of the parent plant aaBbNn. aBN aBn abN abn 1 2 3 ar | || aBN aBn abN abn “DNI BN | ON | bX | BN | 5 6 7 8 || aBN aBn abN abn aBn aBn aBn aBn aBn 2) Cae 11 12 abN | aBN aBn abN abn ~ i) @bN abN abN abN | 13 14 15 16 | aba | aBN aBn abN abn | abn abn abn abn i | pea eee Sa } Fig. 11. DIAGRAM TO ILLUSTRATE THE NATURE OF THE OFFSPRING OF @ C.... Setchelli (aaBbNn). Since self-fertilization is the rule in Capsella, it was an easy matter to test the validity of the theory. A form aaBbNn, one with unelongated primary lobes, sinuses reaching the midrib and with early leaves of a ‘‘narrow’’ type should yield, on self-fertilization, the following com- binations: 1.bbnn (square 16), a plant of which, accord- ing to our definition, the earlier roset leaves should be No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 211 broad and of which the later leaves shall lack incisions reaching to the midrib, a plant, in short, which should have all the characteristics of Shull’s simplex. Further- more, on being selfed, it should yield a uniform offspring, in all respects resembling the parent. Such plants actually were encountered. Of the plants grown to maturity, twelve were selected as seed-bearers. All bore the simplex character. Ten of these plants were selected from among the first generation of plants of the supposed zygotic constitution BbNn, while one parent (yielding No. 25,712) was derived from a plant bearing the simplex character and another (yielding No. 31,112) was derived from a plant which was shown to have the zygotic constitution bbNn. TABLE I EVIDENCE OF HomMozyGoTic CHARACTER OF Simplex (bbnn) | | Character of a, a tg Index Number of Parent Grand- Parent parent 25,712 78 Gis 8,112BR12P9 bbnn bbnn 25,912 22 G 8,212BR3P1 bbnn BbNn 26,312 4 G 8,212CR5P1 bbnn BbNn 26,512 187 G 8,212F R3P3 bbnn BbNn 26,712 G 8,212HR7P7 bbnn BbNn 30,012 276 O 8,212CR5P1 bbnn BbNn 30,112 108 O 8,212H R2 bbnn BbNn 30,212 Oo 8,212GR6P8 bbnn BbNn 30,312 162 G ,212GR6P8 bbnn BbNn 30,712 oO 8,212HR3P6 bbnn BbNn 31,112 50 Oo 26,012AR7P6 bbnn bbNn 3,113 207 O 26,912BR1P3 _ bbnn | BbNn_ 1,399 | This table offers an excellent illustration of the small danger of an accidental cross, even if the plants are not guarded, always, of course, when the proper precautions, indicated above, are taken. Numbers 26,312 and 30,012, as well as numbers 30,212 and 30,312, respectively, offer instances of uniform inheritance in plants possessing recessive characters only and of which the parents in the one case were left unguarded, in the other caged. Had 15 In this column ‘‘G’’ indicates that the parent plant was guarded, “O” that the plant was open- -fertilized. In other tables the same abbre- Viation will be used. 212 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII crossing taken place in the case of the unguarded flowers, this would, because of the purely recessive characters possessed by simplex, have become apparent at once. In all cases the parents were checked by means of herbarium specimens or photographs, or both. 2. bbNN (square 11). According to our hypothesis, a plant of this zygotic construction should have the earlier roset leaves narrow and the climax leaves should lack incisions to the midrib. It also should breed true. A plant fulfilling these conditions has not been encountered, or rather, its recognition was delayed until the offspring of the corresponding heterozygote bbNn could be observed. As will be shown, the zygotic combination bbNN yields a plant with the external characteristics of arachnoidea. 3. bbNn (squares 12 and 15). A plant of this zygotic constitution should have narrow early leaves and the climax leaves should lack incisions to the midrib. On self-fertilization it should yield 25 per cent. bbNN, 50 per cent. bbNn and 25 per cent. bbnn. bN bn bN bN bN bn bn bn Several plants were found which fulfilled the require- ments as to leaf characters. Such plants, on being selfed, yielded approximately 25 per cent. simplex, which we know to have the zygotic constitution bbnn, while about 50 per cent. bore the parental characters, supposedly rep- resented by bbNn. The remaining 25 per cent. clearly belonged to the type arachnoidea. In all, 12 plants were selected as seed-bearers, some being guarded, others re- maining uncaged. The results are given in Table II. The totals closely approximate the Mendelian ratio, yielding, respectively, bbNN 24 per cent., bbNn 49 per cent. and bbnn 27 per cent. Having established the identity of bbnn (simplex) and bbNn (attenuata), we are forced to recognize bbNN as the zygotic construction of arach- noidea. It would be a comparatively easy matter to test No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 213 this directly, provided the form arachnoidea produced seed. Though I have grown several hundreds of these plants, I have obtained in all but eight seeds, and these as the result of hybridization. Hence the test must be made indirectly through crossing of forms yielding the desired gametic combinations. TABLE II EVIDENCE OF HETEROZYGOTIC CHARACTER OF attenuata (bbNn) Number of Plants | | | | 7 bcs Char. Index es ON N bb Nn bbnn Char. | G : Num- E Index Number A OP o | - w $ w v os 5 of Parent Parent |0 Gran “ a 'o fz] © =| 1o | paren (de (3/48 | 2 | as | m £ | = 2 es 2 26,012| 19 | 25.50| 56| 51.00| 27| 25.50 eel bbNn | G | BbNn 26,412| 8| 7.50} 14| 15.00] 8| 7.50 DR1OP8 | bbNn | G | BbNn 31,212| 16| 14.75| 27 | 29.50| 16 | 14.75 26'0124R2P1 bbNn | O | bbNn 31,312| 22 | 15.00) 19 f 30.00] 19 | 15.00 | 26,012AR1P2 | bbNn | O | bbNn 31,412) 10 | 14.25) 25 | 28.50| 22 | 14.25 | 26,012AR1P3 | bbNn | O | bbNn | 47 | 51.00| 3 50 R 31,512) 24 | 25.50! 47| 51. 1 | 25.50 | 26,012AR1P4 |bbNn | O |bbNn 31,612; 2 .50 | j 4 0 | 26,012AR6 bbNn | O | bbNn 31,812} 26 | 28.25) 54| 56.50| 33 | 28.25 | 26,012BR1P3 | bbNn | O | bbNn 31,912| 11 | 16.50) .00| 15 | 16.50} 26,012BR1P6 |bbNn | O |bbNn 3,213| 61 | 64.25 130 |128.50| 66 25 | 26,912DR2P4 |bbNn | O | BbNn 3,313} 70 | 61.50| 117 |123.00| 59 | 61.50 | 26,912ER6P6 Nn | O | BbNn 3,513| 27 | 35.50! 71| 71.00| 34 | 35.50 | 26,912FR6P4 | bbNn |O | BbNn Total .| 296 [308.50 | 604 (617.00 | 334 (308.50 Of the twelve parent plants concerned in the above ex- periment, five were selected from among the first genera- tion of a plant having the supposed zygotic constitution BbNn, while seven were the direct offspring of No. 26,012, which had been shown to yield the three forms, arach- noidea, attenuata and simplex, as indicated in Table II. The simplex, obtained by selfing a plant of bbNn, breeds true, as indicated in Table I, No. 31,112, a simplex, yield- ing a uniform simplex offspring, consisting of 50 indi- viduals. 4. BBnn (square 6). A plant of this supposed zygotic constitution should resemble, in all respects, Shull’s rhomboidea, the earliest roset leaves being broad, and the incisions of the climax leaves reaching the midrib. It- Should breed true. Five lots, involving four parents, were grown. Again it was shown, in the case of No. 26,812 and 214 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII No. 30,612, that the fact that plants are left unguarded does not affect results. The parents, in all cases, were selected from among the first generation of plants having the supposed zygotic constitution BbNn. The results are given in Table ITI. TABLE III EVIDENCE OF HoMozyeoric CHARACTER OF rhomboidea (BBnn) Index Number No. of Plants Index Number of Parent Char. of Parent Goro 25,812 20 8,212BR1P6 BBnn G 26,812 80 8,412BR3P2 BBnn G 27,012 6 8,412ER10P6 BBnn G 27,112 210 8,412FR13P12 BBnn G 30,612 96 8,412BR3P2 BBnn O In all cases- the off- spring was uniformily of the rhomboidea character. Bn Bn Bn bn Bn bn bn bn 5. Bbnn (squares 8 and 14). Plants of this zygotic constitution should resem- ble those of the preceding group, but on being selfed should yield 25 per cent. homozygotie rhomboidea (BBnn), 50 per cent. het- erozygotic rhomboidea (Bbnn) and 25 per cent. simplex (bbnn). These three forms were found to constitute the Fic. 12. Crax Leaves or a Her- Offspring of a single plant, omenie i p bei ee i San eee spring of a plant of the supposed zygotic constitution BbNn. This plant, from 16 Bursa... 39. No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 215 the first, was classified as a rhomboidea. At the present time, a photograph of the young roset confirms this classification. But two climax leaves, which, in the earlier part of these experiments, were deemed suff- cient, show that the sinuses do not quite reach the midrib (Fig. 12). Unfortunately, Shull, in the description of his No. 054.28,% does not mention this point, though he does point out that ‘‘the later rosette-leaves had some of the secondary lobes acutish, but not elongated.’’? In the older climax leaves, even of a homozygous rhom- boidea, I find that the secondary lobes disappear. Shull, in the description just referred to, is so specific as to the typical rhomboidea character of the heterozygote that I have hesitated to classify the heterozygotes and the homo- zygotes. But the homozygotie rhomboidea, obtained as the extracted recessive of a selfed plant of the supposed zygotic constitution BBNn, always has sinuses which reach the midrib. In other combinations, also, one can distinguish between BB and Bb by the relative depth of the sinus. For the present, then, we will rely upon this character. In the case under discussion (26,612, the off- spring of 8,212HR1P3, guarded) there were among the 39 plants 6 which clearly were simplex, the heterozygotic rhomboidea was represented by 22 individuals, and the homozygotic rhomboidea by 11 individuals, the calculated ratio being 9.75:19.50:9.75. The percentage of simplex is far too low, 15.4 per cent., instead of 25 per cent., but, considering the small number of individuals concerned, the total outcome is fairly satisfactory. It is almost un- necessary to add that in this, as in other cases, the off- spring of the various plants is being tested as fast as time and facilities permit. Type 4.—Having shown the presumable correctness of our supposition as to the zygotic constitution of the initial plant (BbNn), as far as the presence, appearance and be- havior on breeding of simplex, rhomboidea and attenuata are concerned, there remains to identify the major group of combinations which, in a simple di-polyhybrid, constitutes 216 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII nine sixteenths of the total offspring and may be uniform in appearance, the constituents being separable only by breeding, ‘‘eine heillose Arbeit,’? as Baur has it. For- tunately, in this case, it is possible to distinguish readily between the various combinations. One of the combinations, BBNN (square 1), should breed true, being homozygotic for both characters con- cerned. We would expect such a plant to have narrow first leaves and climax leaves with incisions to the mid- rib. Thus far I have not encountered such a plant, some- thing which at one time led me to consider the possibility of gametic repulsion, in this instance the gamete BN being incapable of existence. This supposition seemed the more plausible since the two genes B and N well might be supposed to be antagonistic, the one being responsible for an incision of the leaf to the midrib, the other tending to make the leaf, especially the earlier leaves, narrow. Were this assumption correct, none of the zygotic combi- nations found in squares 1, 2 and 5, 3 and 9, and 4 and 13, would be formed, though we would expect the same com- bination as occurs in squares 4 and 13 to make its appear- ance as the result of the fusion of the gametes bN and Bn (squares 7 and 10). Were this supposition correct, we should have a case similar to that of the sweet pea ‘‘ Purple Invincible,’’ and we could not expect the gamete (bn) to be formed. Since, however, simplex (bbnn) appears in our cultures, this theory must be rejected. Recently also, in culture No. 30,412, an instance was found in which the guarded parent, supposedly of type 4, yielded, not simplex, rhomboidea, attenuata, arachnoidea as well as the parental type, but only arachnoidea, rhomboidea and the parental type, and in proportions closely approximating a ratio 1:1:2. A plant which yielded 25 per cent. rhomboidea and no simplex, must have been homozygotie for B, and since it yielded also 50 per cent. of type 4, must have been hetero- zygotic for N, its zygotic constitution therefore being BBNn. Such a plant, on self-fertilization, should yield 25 per cent. rhomboidea. Provided the homozygote and No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 217 the heterozygote have the same appearance, the remaining 75 per cent. should resemble tle parent (Fig. 9, b). BN | Bn BN | BN | | | BN | Bn [2 Bae: St ae But in one case (30,412), the parent being 8,412BR9P9, and open fertilized, the offspring consisted of 26.3 per cent. rhomboidea, 46.2 per cent. of the parental type and 27.5 per cent. arachnoidea. If our supposition as to the zygotic constitution of the parent is correct, then the zygotic constitution of the arachnoidea in this offspring must be BBNN. In the case of a selfed attenuata, we found that approximately 25 per cent. of the offspring was composed of arachnoidea of the probable zygotic con- stitution bbNN. Is it possible that any Capsella, homo- zygotic for N, would have the appearance of arachnoidea? This seems more than probable, and other evidence, to be adduced later, appears to support this view. The history of the BBNn is as follows: During 1912 I grew No. 8,412 from seeds of a plant which resembled the grandparent 4,108.6. It was com- posed of 1,079 individuals, among which various types, such as ‘‘broad,’’ ‘‘narrow’’ and ‘‘linear,’’ could be rec- ognized. Not all plants were thus classified, a fourth group of ‘‘intermediates’”’ being formed, indicating that some of the plants, while in certain respects resembling simplex and especially rhomboidea (deep lobing, second- ary lobes), in other characters more closely approximated the “narrows,” since their early leaves had been noted as “‘narrow.’’ In the light of recent experience, it is easy to see why the distinction was made, though at the time the conception of the differences was most hazy. Several of these ‘‘intermediates’’ were grown, and of these a single one yielded the seed for the next generation. This plant had been permitted to flower unguarded, but after a number of capsules had developed on the main stalk, this was decapitated and the sideshoots were allowed to de- 218 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII velop. At this time the entire plant was caged. Subse- quently the seeds of the open fertilized and of the guarded flowers were sown separately, with the following results: 30,412. Open Fertilized 80,512. Guarded Plants Plants Per Cent. MOG GON toe re ee oS i Found | Expected Found | Expected Arachnoidea... . 27.5 40 | 36.25 21.15 52 | 61.50 Naw Nas 46.2. 67 | 72.50 36.15 89 | 123 Rhomboidea..... 26.3 38 | 36.25 42.70 105 | 61.50 The figures are given separately to again call attention to the fact that open fertilization is no hindrance to pedi- gree work in Capsella. Since the seeds came from the same parent, we may add the results, which gives us arachnoidea 23.50 per cent., ‘‘narrow’’ 40 per cent. and rhomboidea 36.50 per cent. The fact that the percentage for ‘‘narrow’’ is too low and that for rhomboidea too high, while the percentage for arachnoidea is within the limits of probable error, is probably due to errors in classification, since greater weight was laid upon lobing of the adult leaves than upon comparative width of the earlier ones. The value of this culture lay chiefly in its suggestion of a zygotic combination BBNn, which prior to that time, on account of the gametic repulsion theory, was not supposed to exist. In consequence, a number of cultures were made, with the following result: TABLE IV EVIDENCE OF HETEROZYGOTIC CHARACTER OF Treleaseana (BBNn) _ Number of Plants BBNN BBNn BBnn Char. Index Index No. of Char. | O of No. Parent Ex- pected Found Ex- pected Found Ex- pected 38 6 26 | 30 30,412AR2P6 |B 3,913 | 36 | 44.75) 87 | 89.50 56 | 44.75) 30,412AR4P3 | BBNn 14 | 15.25} 28 | 30.50; 19 | 15.25) 30,412AR6P3 | BBNn 15 | 28.50! 65 34 | 28.50 | 30,412BR2P6 |B. 4,313 | 33 | 33 62 | 66 37 | 33 30,412BR6P5 | BBNn 4,413 | 37 | 45.25| 102 | 90.50; 42 | 45.25) 30,412BR9P2 | BBNn w = Oooo w & 5 Total .| 173 [196.75 | 400 (393.50 | 214 (196.75 No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 219 The ‘‘narrows’’ in question, then, fulfilled our expecta- tion on the basis of a zygotic constitution BBNn. In some cases the percentages are too high, in others too low. The total yields fairly satisfactory results, to wit: BBNN 22 per cent., BBNn 51 per cent. and BBnn 27 per cent. Two tests of the extracted recessive, a homozygotic rhom- boidea, were made. The cultures, No. 3,713, from a guarded rhomboidea (30,412AR2P3) and No. 4,113, from an unguarded rhomboidea (30,412AR8P3), both derived from plants of the supposed zygotic. constitution BBNn, _ yielded, respectively, 54 and 207 plants, all of which bore the typical rhomboidea characters. In the cultures just tabulated, the plants of the sup- posed zygotic constitution BBNn resembled the parent in all respects. The form arachnoidea, in this case, must have the zygotic formula BBNN. Unfortunately, in this case also, it proved unfertile. A better acquaintance with plants of the zygotic consti- tution BBNn led us to formulate certain differences be- tween them and our original ‘‘narrow.’’ Plants of the BBNn character, readily can be segregated from those of the BbNn character by somewhat narrower primary lobes, split to the midrib and the development, in climax leaves of well-grown specimens, of a secondary lobe, not pronounced but recognizable (Figs. 8, 9). On the basis of these morphological differences, as well as because of the behavior of the plant on breeding, I propose to segregate it from type 4 under the name X Capsella Bursa-pastoris Treleaseana. This form is homozygotic for B, while Setchelliana is heterozygotic for B. Both are heterozygotic for N. They may be ex- pected to look alike during the early stages. Later they Show a difference, since the form containing Bb does not develop sinuses as deep as the form containing BB. The form Treleaseana, when young, can readily be distin- guished from a heterozygotic rhomboidea (Bbnn) by the relative width of the early leaves; later such a distinction is difficult (Figs. 4,5, 7). If any distinction at all is to be made, it should be made on the basis of the rounding 220 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII of the lobes, those of T’releaseana being sharp, those of the heterozygotie rhomboidea rounded. I am fully aware that in thus naming genotypes, I am departing from all rules laid down by systematists. But a rule is useful only as long as it serves a purpose. For the geneticist, the rules of systematists are of small value. Subspecies, variety, form, are, after all, but very general terms, almost incapable of definition because of too fre- quent abuse. But once we have determined the zygotic con- stitution of any plant, we have placed ourselves on a firmer basis. Behavior in breeding is the proper criterion. And while I recognize that this, for systematic purposes, is impracticable, at the same time I assert the right to use a trinomial for any organism of known zygotic constitu- tion, this being, at the present time at least, the easiest way of designating any particular form. Some day we shall have formulas, corresponding to those of chemistry, to designate the lesser forms. The increase in the number of named forms, a neces- sary consequence, need cause no alarm, since they concern only him who occupies himself with one species exclu- sively. But we must go even further than this. Squarely facing the issue, we find ourselves placed in a position which necessitates the naming of heterozygotes. Obvi- ously, numerous objections could be urged. But since it has been shown, on the one hand, that certain forms can exist only in a heterozygous form (Baur’s Antirrhinum) and, on the other, that not only the difference between the homozygote and the heterozygote is as great as that be- tween many of our ‘‘systematic’’ species (for instance, attenuata, bbNn, and arachnoidea, bbNN), but that a homozygotic condition for a single gene gives the same result, whatever the condition of the other known genes, at least as thus far determined (arachnoidea occurs as aaBBNN, aaBbNN and aabbNN), the advantage of nam- ing all forms of different zygotic constitution must be granted. : Thus far we have not encountered a plant of the zygotic constitution BbNN, at least as far as can be judged from No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 221 breeding experiments. On being selfed such a plant should yield: BN | bN BN | BN BN | oN bN | bN It has been shown that plants of the zygotic constitu- tion BBNN and bbNN exhibit the arachnoidea type. At least 50 per cent. of the offspring then should show this character. But if the suggestion made above is the cor- rect one, i. e., that all plants homozygotic for N exhibit the arachnoidea type, then the parent and its entire off- spring should bear this character. The unfortunate in- fertility of arachnoidea prevents us from submitting this hypothesis to direct experimental proof. But there exist indirect means for establishing the probable truth of our contention. In the first place, we may cross two plants, the identity of which can be established beyond doubt, to wit, attenuata (bbNn) and Treleaseana (BBNn). Sucha cross would yield: BN Bn bN Of these, we would recognize Bbnn because of its rhom- boidea character, 50 per cent. would be recognized as Setchelliana (BbNn), while the remainder, if our surmise is correct, would consist of arachnoidea. Experiments to determine this are under way. At the present we have another, though by far less accurate, means of testing our hypothesis. If the combination NN always results in a form arachnoidea, the offspring of a plant of the zygotic constitution BbNn would be composed of : 4 Setchelliana (BbNn), 2 Treleaseana (BBNn), - 2 attenuata (bbNn), 4 arachnoidea (1 BBNN, 2 BONN, 1 bbNN), 222 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII 3 rhomboidea (1 BBnn, 2 Bbunn), 1 simplex (bbnn). Since BbNn, BBNn and bbNn, in the earlier experi- ments, might have been confounded in the later stages, and since there is little doubt as to the earlier stages, these three forms have been combined in Table V. TABLE V RESULTS FROM SELECTED Setchelliana (BbNn) “ Narrow ” | arachnoidea rhomboidea simplex Index | EES EROS eee ro Le Found | Expected | Found | Expected | Found | Expected | Found | Expected 26,912 | 134 | 157.6 | 94 | 78.8 | 68 | 59.1 19 | 19.7 3,613 94 89 45 45 33 33.75 8 11.25 This, especially in the case of No. 3,613, is a fairly close approximation to what we might expect. When in No 3,613 we attempt to distinguish between Setchelliana, Treleaseana and attenuata, we get the following num- bers, the expected numbers following in parentheses: BbNn 39(45), BBNn 21(22.50), bbNn 34(22.50), the last number being far too high. When the experiments were begun, we distinguished only between ‘‘narrow,’’ ‘“broad’”’ and ‘‘linear.’’ To-day we know that the ‘‘nar- rows” include Treleaseana, Setchelliana and attenuata, that the ‘‘broads”’ include rhomboidea and simplex, while the linears are identical with arachnoidea. In this light it is of interest to go back to the first generation of 1910. Our data yield the figures given in Table VI. TABLE VI “ Narrow ” “ Linear” | “ Broad ” Index No. pe | | | Found Expected | Found Expected | Found Expected 7,911 | 34 oa 9 15.25 | 18 15.25 8,111 ba SF 35.50 16 mwi B 17.75 8,311 | 66 61 Ye 30.50 | 24 30.5 8,711 | page 13.50 | 14 13.50 8,811 | 49 46.50 20 98.55: — 24 23.25 9,011 | 4 7.50 4 3.75 | 7 3.7. 9,511 t oe 6 +o 38 Let 38 voni 28 23.50 | 5 176 |. l 11.75 Total i... 38 308.50 | 133 154.25 _156 154.25 Per cent........ | 63.2 50 LS 25 | gee 25 No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 223 It must be granted that the approximation is fairly close, and that, taken in consideration. with the others, it offers ample support for the correctness of the diagnosis of the zygotic constitution of the original plant. It at least offers a working basis. One would be tempted to accept it as a final solution were it not for the fortunate appearance of a plant which does not fit into our scheme and which, provisionally, has been named Capsella Bursa- pastoris orbicularis. CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ORBICULARIS “This form differs from any other plant encountered in my cultures. While in a general manner resembling simplex, it differs in being more robust, having larger flowers (though not as large as those of C. grandiflora), and in having orbicular first leaves (Fig. 13). All leaves are covered with stout hairs. S) Lk) It is a plant which tempts us Y- to draw a parallel between it CTE A and @Œnothera gigas, a name A which I have not used for the sake of avoiding an implied comparison. The first plant of this type appeared in a culture of attenuata (26,012BR3P5) and was of sufficiently striking appearance, though but four or five leaves had developed, to call for a special note and a photograph. Later the plant was potted and finally seed was gathered from the unguarded plant. From this seed four seedlings were obtained. At least three of them closely resembled the parent, the fourth having somewhat narrower leaves. Later the differences between these plants and those of simplex became more apparent (Fig. 14). Those of my students to whom the differences have been pointed out have not the slightest difficulty in distinguishing between the two forms. It is hoped that later, when by means of prolonged cultures I shall have made myself more familiar with this form, it may be made the subject of a distinct paper where histological and cytological studies will find Fig. 13. chorea oF C. . . orbicul 224 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII a place. One would be inclined to look upon orbicularis as a mutation. But the fact that at first we classed arachnoidea as such, later to prove it of hybrid origin,'* Fic. 14. Four SEEDLINGS oF C. .. . orbicularis AND (THE LOWER) Two SEED- LINGS OF O. . . . simplez. would tend to make us cautious, and lead us to attempt to find a solution for the origin of orbicularis in the disso- ciation or combination of certain ‘‘units.’? While I should not care to go quite as far as M. Heribert Nilson’? ‘‘das ganze Mutations phänomen durfte unter einen gemein- samen Gesichtspunkte: der Mendelschen Neukombination eingeordnet werden können,” yet it is probable that here the majority of alleged mutations may be classed. 17 Baur’s (Vererbungslehre, 189) narrow-leaved Melandrium album is perhaps susceptible of the same explanation. 18 Zeitschr. f. ind. Abst. u. Bererb., 8: 89, 1912. No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 225 An examination of the herbarium material placed at my disposal reveals the fact that plants, apparently identical with C. orbicularis, occur in Europe. In the Engelmann herbarium of the Missouri Botanical Garden are two sheets (No. 3,661 and 3,664) containing specimens which undoubtedly must be classed here. The latter sheet bears the label: Thlaspi Bursa-pastoris humile. Heidel- berg. April 1828. A culture of Capsella, derived from seed of a single plant, unfortunately not preserved, escaped from culti- vation in the Experiment Garden, and consisting of 182 individuals (Ehlers, No. 4,813), appears to be composed entirely of orbicularis. And while I have never encoun- tered the plant in nature, these two facts lead us to another possible explanation. Perhaps the appearance of orbic- ularis in the original culture was due to an accidental admixture, such as is almost impossible to guard against when experimental plants are grown in a greenhouse used for a variety of purposes. The exact relation which orbicularis bears to the other types of Capsella here described can, of course, be de- termined only after a series of experiments has been car- ried out. However, the delay in the completion of the manuscript, caused by the unfortunate destruction, by fire, of the botanical laboratories of the University of Michigan, enables me to add that a third generation of orbicularis, the parent being No. 32,012R1P3, shows at least two and possibly three types, of which one is espe- cially interesting in having rather narrow leaves, at least as compared with those of typical orbicularis. The con- trast between the two forms is increased by the fact that in the narrow-leaved form the foliage is entirely glabrous, while in the typical orbicularis the leaves are covered with numerous stiff, almost bristle-like, hairs. X CAPSELLA Bursa-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA By this name is designated the linear-leaved form, the appearance of which induced us to undertake the cultiva- tion of Capsella Bursa-pastoris Setchelliana. 226 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (VoL. XLVIII Fic. 15. tOSETS ILLUSTRATING TEE Two TYPES Already the leaves which immediately follow the coty- ledons serve to distinguish plants of this type from all others. At the ten-leaf stage even the casual observer is able to segregate them at once from the other rosets. The leaves are acicular and the cotyledons far larger than those of the seedlings of the other forms. The greater size of the cotyledons may be attributed to the insufficiency of the subsequent leaves. If one removes the terminal bud of seedlings of Atri- plex hortensis or one of its color varieties, it will be found that the cotyledons increase in length far beyond normal, sometimes reaching a length of 8 em. Under favorable conditions the leaves of X C. arachnoidea may reach a length of 100 mm., with a greatest width of 6 mm. (Fig. 16). The stem ordinarily is weak, having a diameter of only 1 mm. It may reach a length of 30 em. (Fig. 17)- No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 227 THUS FAR RECOGNIZED 1N C... . orbicularis. The flowers are small, the petals especially so. The anthers shrivel up early and as a rule are devoid of pollen grains. Occasionally a few can be demonstrated. The ovary, though small, contains what appear to be ovules capable of being fertilized. Thus far I have collected eight seeds contained in 6 capsules on unguarded plants of arachnoidea (Fig. 18). Two of these germinated, the one yielding a plant whieh looks like simplex, though having a lar ge amount of red coloring matter in the peti- oles, while the other is an arachnoidea. Attempts to arti- ficially fertilize arachnoidea have failed absolutely. As has been shown above, one may distinguish, on the basis of genotypic constitution, three forms of arach- noidea, viz.: BBNN, BbNN and bbNN. Externally no 228 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII differences can be noted. A single exception perhaps may be made to this statement. It had been noted that speci- mens of arachnoidea frequently showed fasciation. This fasciation seems most marked in plants of the zygotic constitution BBNN (Figs. 19, 20, 21). Fic. 16. Roser or æ CC... . arachnoidea. While it is hoped that later a more extended report may be made upon this plant, at present it may be stated that there exists the probability that it may throw some light upon the nature of fasciations. In earlier publications’® I have brought together some of the known facts bearing upon this teratological character. Though a large por- 19 “í Fasciation in Oxalis crenata and Experimental Production of Fascia- tions,’’? Rep. Mo. Bot, Gard., 17: 147, 1906; ‘‘Fasciations of Known Causation,’’ AMERICAN NATŪRALIST, 42: 81, 1908; ‘‘Inheritance of Fascia- tion in Zea Mays,’’ The Plant World, 14: 1911; ‘‘The Origin of Species in Nature,’’?’ AMERICAN NATURALIST, 45: 641, 1911; ‘‘ Frondescence s Fasciation,’’ Plant World, . 2910. Passion in Ozalis crenata,’ Botanical Journal, 2: 111, 1913. No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 229 tion of the experimental garden is devoted to cultures of fasciated races, nothing further has been determined than that the fasciated character is inherited, that it is trans- mitted through non-fasciated individuals, that its ap- parentness depends upon nutrition, that it behaves as a fÍ Fig. 17. Two HERBARIUM SPECIMENS OF @ ©... . arachnoidea. recessive character and that the fasciated character of the stem appears to be associated with split leaves and cup- Shaped leaves. Ina paper read before the Research Club of the University of Michigan on March 16, 1910, and an- nounced under the title ‘‘The Identity and Inheritance of Teratological Characters,” I showed that split leaves, ascidia, certain disturbances in the arrangement of the 230 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII flowers, supernumerary locules in the fruit, etc., may safely be taken as an indication of the presence of the fasciated character. More recently, Kajanus,” working Fic. 18. SHOOT or gz C.... oe a WITH A beet NUMBER OF INFERTILE AND FE ERTILE CAPSU with different material, has fully confirmed the views which I expressed at the time. This is of particular in- 20 Kajanus, B. Sieh seins r Fasziation bei Trifolium pratense L.,’’ Zeitsch. f. ind. Abst. u. Vererb., T: 63, 1912; ‘‘Ueber einige vegetative Anomalien bei cafes Siac L., ibid., 9: 111, 1913. No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 231 terest in connection with Capsella arachnoidea, since many of the plants which do not show a fasciated stem do show split leaves (Fig. 20) and a most peculiar whorl- ing of the flowers (Fig. 21). Fic. 19. FASCIATED PLANT OF ¢ C. .. . arachnoidea. The spatulate condition of the leaves of the seedling shown in Fig. 2 is believed to have been due to fasciation. Capsella Bursa-pastoris arachnoidea, then, bears all the earmarks of a fasciated race. All of the three zygotic combinations which yield the arachnoidea type are homo- zygotic for N. The recent work of East and Hayes, and of Emerson on Zea Mays has shown that the fasciated 232 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Vou. XLVIII + ') Cal YJ i \ | | j Fig. 21. ABNORMAL WHORLED ARRANGEMENT OF CENCES OF @ 0. ... a í Frio: 20. LEAVES oF #0... arachnoidea. TE y VA j4 an r =% * BS ri { f TF A ` tf? } Lf Dri Y SAF Xo aA , J X “i a THE FLOWERS IN INFLORES- arachnoidea. No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 233 character is dominant, though Mendel, in his experiments with Pisum umbellatum, has shown it to be recessive. No fasciation, thus far at least, has been noted in the other forms used in these experiments. SUMMARY $ A culture of Capsella Bursa-pastoris proved heterozy- gotic, yielding certain new forms (X C. Bursa-pastoris Setchelliana, XC.Bursa-pastoris Treleaseana, XC.Bursa- pastoris arachnoidea and X C. Bursa-pastoris attenuata), as well as certain forms already described by Shull (C. Bursa-pastoris rhomboidea and C. Bursa-pastoris sim- plex) in the proportion 4:2:4:2:3:1. The distinction between simplex and rhomboidea, both inter se and be- tween them and the other forms, is readily made by any one familiar with Shull’s investigations. These two plants agree in having the earlier leaves broad (Fig. 4). The climax leaves of rhomboidea and simplex show marked differences, especially. as far as the incision of the blade is concerned. These incisions, in simplex, reach a depth equal to approximately one fourth of the width of the blade (Fig. 10). In rhomboidea the incisions are deeper, reaching the midrib in the homozygous form (Fig. 12). The leaves of the latter also show marked secondary lobes. The distinction between X C. Bursa-pastoris Setchelli- ana, X C. Bursa-pastoris Treleaseana and X C. Bursa- pastoris attenuata is made with greater difficulty. They agree in having long and narrow first leaves. The climax leaves of Treleaseana and Setchelliana show marked incisions, exceeding one fourth of the width of the blade, and which may reach the midrib (Fig. 9). The latter form also may show marked secondary lobes. Besides the phenotypes here mentioned occur two others, the one, X C. Bursa-pastoris orbicularis, with an almost orbicular first leaf (Fig. 13) and a climax leaf greatly resembling that of simplex (Figs. 14, 15), though differing i in texture. This form has not been sufficiently studied, but is believed to be identical with one known to 234 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [VoL XLVIII occur in Europe. Finally there is X C. Bursa-pastoris arachnoidea, a sterile, linear-leaved form, with a weak stem and which frequently shows fasciation (Figs. 17-21). To facilitate a distinction between these forms, a key is appended: a. Early leaves broads b. Early leaves orbicular. orbicularis. bb. Early leaves twice as long as broad. c. Climax leaves incised to midrib. rhomboidea. cc. Early leaves not incised to midrib. simplex. aa. Early leaves long and narrow. b. Early leaves acicular. arachnoidea. bb. Early leaves 24-3 times as long as broad. c. Climax leaves not incised to midrib. attenuata, ce. Climax leaves incised to or almost to the midrib. d. Secondary lobes pronounced. Treleaseana. dd. Secondary lobes absent. Setchelliana. It was found that, besides the genes A, B, C and D, whose existence was shown by Shull, there exists another gene, N, responsible for the narrow character of the earlier leaves. For the various forms, mentioned here, the following zygotic constitutions have been tentatively determined: simplex, bbnn; rhomboidea; BBnn and Bbnn; Setchelliana, BbNn; Treleaseana, BBNn; attenuata, bbNn; arachnoidea, BBNN, BbNN and bbNN. The zygo- tic constitution of orbicularis has not been determined. As to the probable origin of X C. Bursa-pastoris Setch- elliana, little can be said. It most probably results from a cross between rhomboidea and attenuata (BBnn X bbNn). This seems the most plausible explanation since, judging from herbarium specimens, both attenuata and rhom- boidea occur throughout the United States. Unfortu- nately such an assumption necessitates an explanation of the origin of attenuata. My thanks are due to the regents of the University of Michigan for the facilities placed at my disposal, to head- gardener Adolph Weiner for his constant care of the ex- perimental plants, to Messrs. J. H. Ehlers, A. Povah, C. Oberlin and A. W. Murdock for assistance in classifica- tion of the seedlings and to the director of the Missouri Botanical Garden for the loan of herbarium material. No. 568] X CAPSELLA BURSA-PASTORIS ARACHNOIDEA 285 CONCLUSIONS 1. Besides the genes (4, B, C, D) discovered by Shull, there exists in Capsella a gene N, responsible for the nar- row character of the early leaves of certain forms. 2. Absence of the gene N results in the formation of early leaves of a ‘‘broad”’ character. 3. The form designated arachnoidea is of hybrid origin, as are the forms Setchelliana, Treleaseana and attenuata. 4. X Capsella Bursa-pastoris arachnoidea is formed whenever the plant is homozygotic for N, whatever the constitution of the remainder of the zygote (BBNN, BbNN, bbNN), i. e., a homozygous condition for the pres- ence of a single factor may overshadow the influence of others. 5. Homozygocity for a single factor may be responsible for total, or almost total, sterility. 6. A knowledge of the early stages, as well as of the climax leaves, is essential for the classification of the phenotypes of Capsella Bursa-pastoris. BIOLOGY OF THE THYSANOPTERA. II DR. A. FRANKLIN SHULL UNIVERSITY OF MICHIGAN II. SEX AND THE LIFE CYCLE INTRODUCTION From observations made on the abundance of males in several species, Jordan (1888) was led to believe that there might be among Thysanoptera, as in aphids, an alternating life cycle; that is, that there might be a series of parthenogenetic generations during the summer, fol- lowed by a generation of males and sexual females in the latter part of the summer or in the fall. Coupled with this he suspected that there were winged forms in the parthenogenetic part of the cycle, and at least occasional wingless individuals in the sexual phase. Uzel (1895), however, was unable to detect any indica- tions of such a cycle. He held that there could be no question of parthenogenesis in a species in which males were abundant all the time or at intervals. Only in spe- cies in which the males were too rare to impregnate all the females would he admit parthenogenesis. To prove, in such a species, an alternating cycle like that of the aphids, it must, in Uzel’s opinion, be shown that the males are abundant only at certain seasons. As Uzel was ac- quainted with no European species in which males were plentiful at but one season, he rejected Jordan’s sugges- tion regarding an alternating cycle, and his view seems to have been accepted by thysanopterists since that time. To Uzel’s argument it may be objected that the pres- ence of males, and even the occurrence of copulation, is no proof that parthenogenesis is wanting. For among the aphids and rotifers, the parthenogenetic and sexual females exist side by side. Nor is parthenogenesis in these two groups facultative (optional), as Uzel appears to assume for Thysanoptera; a female is either only 236 No. 568] BIOLOGY OF THE THYSANOPTERA 237 sexual or only parthenogenetic. Moreover, in the roti- fers, females incapable of fertilization copulate as fre- quently as do those requiring fertilization, as was first shown by the work of Maupas (1890) on the rotifer Hydatina. Presence of males and occurrence of copulation are, therefore, no proof of sexual reproduction. But even if we accept, as Uzel does, this criterion of sexuality, Jor- dan’s view that there may be an alternating cycle would receive some support if it could be shown that males are more abundant at one season of the year than at other times. Casual observations made by me several years ago seemed to indicate this seasonal variation in the abund- ance of males. As the data then available were meager, no conclusion was drawn, but I subsequently undertook to obtain such data on a larger scale, by making extensive collections at all seasons of the year to determine the sex ratio. The following pages give these data, along with other observations bearing on sex or the life cycle. I desire to acknowledge the assistance of my wife, by whom much of the labor of determining species and counting the sexes was done. THE Sex RATIO IN Various Species or THYSANOPTERA In making collections for the purpose of determining the sex ratio, the food plants were examined very care- fully, torn apart if necessary, and every individual cap- tured. This precluded the possibility of obtaining an erroneous sex ratio because one sex was more easily dis- turbed than the other. A few individuals escaped, but they could not have affected the sex ratio very greatly, and it was known from their size that they were some- times of the one sex, sometimes of the other. The sex in the suborder Terebrantia is readily deter- mined by the presence of an ovipositor in the female and the rounded end of the abdomen in the male. In the sub- order Tubulifera, the sex in Anthothrips verbasci was determined by the presence of two short, heavy spines, 238 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII one on each side of the abdomen of the male, near the end. As the specimens, when placed on a microscope slide, nearly always lie either on the dorsal or ventral side, these spines are nearly always readily visible if present. I used this criterion (mentioned in the re-description of the species by Hinds, 1902) only after having taken eleven pairs of this species copulating in nature, and observing in every case that the male possessed these spines, and that in the female they were wanting. In other Tubu- lifera, e. g., Anthothrips niger, sex was determined by the longitudinal chitinous rod in the next to the last abdomi- nal segment of the female. When the specimens were too opaque to observe this rod, they were cleared by boiling in caustic potash. The data from these collections are given in the accom- panying table. Unfortunately the collections could not all be made in one year, nor in the same locality. Those made from July 1 to September 18, 1912, were made at the University of Michigan Biological Station, Douglas Lake, Michigan; all others were made at Ann Arbor, Michigan. It is not probable that the results are greatly modified by collecting in two regions within the state. In this table the larve of all species are combined, as I am unable to distinguish with certainty the larve of several of the species here mentioned. The important facts contained in this table are, it seems to me, the following: Euthrips tritici appeared in spring at first only in the female sex. Males were first collected nearly a month later, and not until about the time fairly large larvæ were found elsewhere. Once the males appear, though their number fluctuates in the individual collections, they fur- nish a fairly constant proportion of the whole number (about one third). The males of Anthothrips verbasci appear in the earli- est collection of this species, and in considerable numbers throughout the season. The total proportion of males is 23 per cent., and the only considerable increases over No. 568] BIOLOGY OF THE THYSANOPTERA 239 this percentage in individual collections are in the three collections made in August, and on October 7. Consider- ing the large majority of females taken September 12, the abundance of males October 7 may be due in some way to TABLE I SHOWING NUMBER OF MALES AND FEMALES OF THE COMMONER SPECIES OF THYSANOPTERA CAPTURED AT INTERVALS DURING THE ACTIVE SEASON Chir Euthrips | aneno an me- | Thrips anapi | Thrips ae trips tritici verbaski | niger | tabaci | striatus Physopus, mani- | Larve, Date | catus «all H Aoa —j— Species Q So ae eee | g Q | P Poo te a Apr. 30, 1911 9| 0 | | May 5 T O | | 10 2- 0 | 17 i 0 | 22 22) 1 2:0 213 24 30| 2/174! 30) 20; 0 | 6 0 7 June 1 41| 17 18 0 6 7 40| 62| 52| 10) 42) O 4| 7 3 15 21| 27| 47| 8 19 0 1 2 25 21 7| 11 410 | 25} 0] 1! 0} O| 1 1 Ti) ô 40 Sa) Piet OS July. 3,1912 | 14| 25 1 0 46 Se 7 zo 20 72 5 3| 0| 82| 29) 2| 0 23 0 1 11 0 3 0 | 95| 18 187) 1! T6 16 7-9 4i 9 T OLI® O 3 17 6| 1 $1] 0 | 42) 0 19 12) 29 2| 0 | 35| 6 5 Ol 3 26 58| 15 2 27 a 0 45! 13 4 29 2 0 4 0 10| 9 12 30 1| 51 42) 15 1 Aug, 5 154/151 28) 10| 1 39 8 1| 30| 20 0,10; 1 9 16 1 0 12 2103) 2 13 26' 48 1 18 60 16 20 i 0 20 18' 17 39| 48 2| 36) 21 40| 3 10 0 13 Sept 60) 7 27| 2 | 60| 33 10 2, 1911 | 35| 15 801190 22 2 88) 1 18 ig 3 15 0 1 o 4 18, 1912 183| 21 Oct. 7,1911 |32 13| 23! 27 19 0 12 4 18} 4 11 0 2; 0 25,1912 | 56] 19 12| 0 15| 4 2 Nov. 9 27 3 12| 0 1 0 E a ee o FoM o 879 441 641 200/162’ 0 226! 0 1530:174 50 | 36 (203/191 240 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII the dying of their food plants; but the greater proportion of males throughout August is probably significant. It should also be stated that I have collected adults of this species, of both sexes, from dead mullein spikes in late winter. Anthothrips niger was found only in the female sex. There are no records of males of this species, so far as I am aware, in any published work. Thrips tabaci was taken almost exclusively in the female sex, the two males found September 2 being the only ones I have ever collected. In Anaphothrips striatus the total number of males is less than 25 per cent. On August 20 and September 2 the proportion of males is considerably greater than 25 per cent., especially on the former date, while at other times the proportion was nearly always less. The collection on August 20 can hardly have been erroneous by chance, for the figures given for that date are combined figures for two collections from different localities. In one of these collections there were 13 females and 14 males, in the other 26 females and 34 males. This strengthens the probability that the excess of males is significant. Thrips physopus was collected in small numbers, but shows a fairly constant proportion of males. Chirothrips manicatus presents curious phenomena. All the collections up to the end of July were made on timothy heads in a small patch a few feet square near the laboratory. On July 11 careful search revealed numer- ous females, but only one male. By July 19 almost all the thrips of this species were gone; only 5 specimens were obtained, and these were females. Less than two weeks later, however (July 30), on other timothy heads in the same small patch, there were found 51 males and but 1 female. No living thrips were taken here later, as the timothy died; but subsequent collections elsewhere, from timothy and bluegrass, show again almost ex- clusively males. No. 568] BIOLOGY OF THE THYSANOPTERA 241 AppitionaL Data BEARING ON THE Lire CYCLE AND Sex In view of the fact, to be discussed later, that Anapho- thrips striatus has hitherto been known almost exclu- sively in the female sex, and is known to reproduce par- - thenogenetically, and the fact that in the collections here recorded the males constitute nearly 25 per cent. of the total, the question arises, are these males functional? If not functional in this species, are the males functional in other species? A number of observations and experi- ments I have made bear on these questions. A single pair of Anaphothrips striatus was found copu- lating in nature, which Uzel would have considered proof that parthenogenesis did not occur. The testes of the males are plainly visible without dissection. Suspecting that they might not be fleshy organs at all, but chitinized structures, perhaps vestiges of testes, I boiled a number of specimens in caustic potash. The testes disappeared, from which I judge they are not merely chitinous bodies. I can say nothing of their cellular nature, owing to the loss of material killed and fixed for that purpose. Nu- merous sections of another species Anthothrips verbasci, however, reveal well-developed testes. Cell divisions (probably the spermatocyte divisions) and nearly mature spermatozoa in bundles were observed in these sections. Though the number of chromosomes could not be deter- mined, it is an interesting fact that spindles in side view usually showed a lagging chromosome. Finally, with further regard to the functioning of males, I have attempted to breed several species par- thenogenetically. The results in the case of Euthrips tritici were so far encouraging that two larve appeared on the plant on which virgin females had been previously placed. But in these cases I could not be certain that the food plant was uninfected. Experiments with Anapho- thrips striatus and Anthothrips verbasci gave negative results, but in each case failure to obtain young by par- thenogenesis may have been due to the conditions. Some observations on the place of pupation may also 242 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII be here recorded. The rarity with which the pupe of most species are discovered in collecting suggested that they might not pupate on the food plant of the larve. Some species of thrips, for example, the pear thrips (Euthrips pyri), are known to pupate in the ground (Moulton, 1912). Since many of the species included in Table I may be found on white clover, which was abun- dant at Douglas Lake, the place of pupation of these species was tested in the following manner. A mass of the flowers of white clover was collected. The flowers were gently squeezed for some time to drive out all the adults. They were then placed in a vessel under cover. After two days, when the flowers were thoroughly dried, they were again gently crushed to make sure that all adults were driven out. At intervals from one to two weeks afterward, 15 adult thrips appeared on the inside of the glass cover. These were of three species, Euthrips tritici, Thrips tabaci and Anthothrips niger. I have also frequently observed the pupe of Antho- thrips verbasci in mullein spikes, those of Sericothrips cingulatus on white clover, the pupa of Trichothrips tri- dentatus under the bark of the white oak, where the larve and adults live, and that of an undescribed species on willow galls along with larve of the same species. I judge from these observations that the majority of thrips pupate on the plants on which the larve live, and that their rarity in collections is due merely to concealment and sluggish habits. Discussion OF THE Resuuts IN RELATION To THE LIFE CYCLE From the data in Table I and the observations given above it is evident that there is considerable diversity in different species with regard to the life cycle, and diver- sity within the same species at different times or in different regions. First, as regards the mode of passing the winter, it would seem that in Euthrips tritici only the females survive that season. The reason for so believing No. 568] BIOLOGY OF THE THYSANOPTERA 243 is that males could not be found in the spring until the females had been active long enough to have produced one generation of offspring. Males occur late in autumn, but must perish before the end of winter. Likewise, neither eggs nor larve live over winter, or larve would appear earlier in spring. In Thrips physopus, on the other hand, males were found as early as the females; hence, in the absence of any collection earlier than May 22, and in ignorance of the time required for develop- ment, I should assume that both sexes survive the winter. Both sexes of Anthothrips verbasci have been seen on dead mulleins in winter. In species, like Euthrips tritici, whose males do not survive the winter, if fertilization of the early spring females takes place at all, it must occur in the fall. I do not regard my breeding experiments as proof of par- thenogenesis in this species, but it is by no means improb- able that parthenogenesis occurs. More rigorous experi- ments are needed. As regards the mode of reproduction during the rest of the year, there is nothing in the sex ratio, as given in Table I, to suggest an alternating cycle in Euthrips tri- tici. In other species, it would be possible to interpret certain facts to mean that an alternation of partheno- genesis and sexual reproduction occurs, or did once occur. There is a well-marked increase in the proportion of males in Anaphothrips striatus, for example, in Au- gust. This is a particularly interesting species. Hinds (1902) saw only the female of this species, though he mounted and examined over a thousand specimens, and he bred it parthenogenetically in the laboratory for months. What purported to be the male was described by Cary (1902), from Maine, but the specimens described were evidently those of another species. The first males ever recorded were described by Shull (1909), two speci- mens among probably two hundred females. It is re- markable, therefore, that in the vicinity of Douglas Lake there should be nearly 25 per cent. of males. Whether 244 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII the presence of numerous males is dependent on climatic conditions, or whether it is a racial difference, there is at present no way of deciding. The weather was unusually cold during the summer in which these records were made, and it is desirable that the effect of temperature be experimentally determined. The presence of males in goodly numbers throughout the summer, the occurrence of copulation in nature, and the failure of an attempt to breed the species parthenogenetically, leave, as the only reason for suspecting that it may have been partheno- genetic at Douglas Lake, the fact that it is parthenoge- netic elsewhere. But if the species is parthenogenetic in one region and sexual in another, it is not difficult to be- lieve that it may be both parthenogenetic and sexual in the same region. It is difficult to decide whether the well- marked increase in the proportion of males in August and early September should be regarded as evidence of such an alternation, or as due to a period of cold weather or other climatic factor, or as a hereditary remnant of the sexual phase of an alternating cycle once possessed by the species. Only experiment, and perhaps cytological study, can decide this question. A similar but less marked increase in the number of males is seen in Anthothrips verbasci, also in August. In that month the proportion of males rose from about 20 per cent. to 40, or even nearly 50 per cent. In this species the increase may be due to the late date at which the first brood of larve becomes mature. The life history of this species is longer than that of most of the suborder Tere- brantia, and may appear to be still longer because ene- mies destroy many of the larger larve. For these rea- sons, in the region of Douglas Lake, the first generation of larve may not become mature until nearly August. If this assumption is correct, the proportion of males found prior to August is the proportion that survive the winter. This explanation receives support from the cytology of the germ cells. As stated above, there is a lagging chro- mosome in the spermatocyte divisions, which suggests No. 568] BIOLOGY OF THE THYSANOPTERA 245 the probability that there are two classes of sperm asso- ciated with sex, as in the bugs and many other animals, and that Sherefoe the sexes should be approximately equal in numbers. The 40 to 50 per cent. of males in August accord fairly well with this explanation. This explanation would not, however, account for the increase in the number of males in late summer in a spe- cies whose life history is much shorter than that of Anthothrips verbasci. Thus, in Anaphothrips striatus, Hinds states that the entire life history is passed through in 12 to 30 days. Even in a cold season, such as that of 1912 at Douglas Lake, therefore, the life history can not have been so long that the first adults would emerge in the middle of August. The increase in the number of males of Anaphothrips in August and September is not to be explained, therefore, as due to the first appearance of a new brood at that time. Thrips tabaci likewise affords interesting, even if mea- ger, evidence regarding the seasonal occurrence of males. In this species males are exceedingly rare. Hinds (1902) redescribed the male in quotation marks, from which it is to be inferred that he did not have specimens. In my own collecting, though the females were quite common, I never saw a male until the summer of 1912. Then two specimens were taken September 2, as shown in Table I. These irregularly occurring males can hardly be fune- tional, so that Thrips tabaci is still probably to be re- garded as wholly parthenogenetic. But their appearance -in late summer may be the vestige of a former sexual phase, and may be caused now, as the sexual phase prob- ably was in part formerly caused, by climatic conditions. Chirothrips manicatus presented, at Douglas Lake, an anomalous condition. As shown in Table I, and stated more explicitly above, females were abundant in a given small area early in July, but practically no males were present. Then, so far as I could determine by painstak- ing collections, the females disappeared; almost no adults of either sex, and not many larve, were to be found. Two 246 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII weeks later, however, males were found in the same area in large numbers. As these males were wingless, they had probably not immigrated. The only other explana- tion that occurs to me is that the larve were present in considerable numbers at the time of the earlier collec- tions, but in the flowers, not among the spikelets of the timothy, so that I did not discover them; and that the female larve reached maturity much earlier than the males. In any case, it is difficult to see how the males can have been functional, when the two sexes occurred at dif- ferent times. If such conditions recur frequently, Chiro- thrips manicatus, even though it produces many males, must be parthenogenetic. SUMMARY The principal conclusions reached in the second part of this work may be stated as follows: Some species of Thysanoptera pass through the winter in both sexes, in others the males perish. In none of those studied does the egg or larva live over winter. Pupation of most of the species of Thysanoptera stud- ied occurs on the food plants where the larve live, not- withstanding that the pupæ seldom appear in collections. From the determination of the sex ratio, Huthrips tritict shows no indication of an alternating life cycle. It is probably sexual throughout the active season, though this is not proven. Chirothrips manicatus occurred abundantly in both sexes, but the two sexes appeared at different seasons. The explanation of this phenomenon is doubtful. An increase in the number of males in Anthothrips verbasci in late summer may be explained as due to the great length of the life history and to selective mortality during the winter, without assuming an alternating life eycle. Anaphothrips striatus, a species which has hitherto been known almost wholly in the female sex, produced about 25 per cent. of males at Douglas Lake. This may No. 568] BIOLOGY OF THE THYSANOPTERA 247 be due either to climatic conditions or to racial differ- ences. Sexual reproduction was not wholly proven, but seems probable. An increase in the number of males in late summer in this species and in Thrips tabaci might be interpreted as indicating a sexual phase, or the vestiges of a sexual phase that existed in the species formerly. Jordan’s belief in an alternating life cycle, which was rejected by Uzel, thus receives some measure of jus- tification. BIBLIOGRAPHY Cary, L. R. 1902. The grass thrips (Anaphothrips striata Osborn). Maine Agr. Exp. Station, Bull. 83, June, pp. 51-82 912 Jones, P. R. 1912. Some new California and Georgia Thysanoptera. U. S. Dept. Agr., Bur. Ent., Tech. Ser. No. 23, Part 1, 24 pp., 7 pls. Jordan, K. 1888. PORS und Biologie der Physapods. Zeit. wiss. Zool., Vol. 47, pp. 541-620. Hinds, W. E. 1902. Contribution to a monograph of e insects of the order ppp top ie & North America. Proc. U. 8. Nat. Museum, Vol. 26, No. , December 20, pp. 79- Maupas, E. 1890. Sur i Te a de 1”Hydatina ata Ehr. Comp. Rend. Acad. Sci. Paris, Tome 111, pp. Moulton, Dudley. 1911. Synopsis, catalog and ‘bihttography of North Aniiesn Thysanoptera. U. S. Dept. Agr., Bur. Ent., Tech. Ser. No. 21, 56 pp. 1912. Papers on deciduous fruit insects and insecticides. IV. The pear thrips and its control. U. S. Dept. Agr., Bur. Ent., Bull. 80, Part IV, pp. 51-66. Shelford, V. E. 1911. Physiological animal geography. Journ. Morph., Vol. 22, No. 3, vig Stns pp. 551-618. Shull, A. F. 1909. me apparently new Thysanoptera from Michigan. Entom. News, a "20, No. 5, pp. 220-228. 1911. A biological survey of the sand dune region = the south shore of Saginaw Bay, Michigan. Thysanoptera and Orthoptera. Mich. Geol. and Biol. Survey, Pub. 4, Biol. Ser. 2, Uzel, H. 1895. Miocrithis der Chiat Piopi. Königgratz, privately published, 482 pp., 1 SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION BARRIERS TO DISTRIBUTION AS REGARDS BIRDS AND MAMMALS THE geographical range of any species of animal may be likened to a reservoir of water in a mountain canyon. The con- fining walls are of varying nature. A concrete dam, absolutely impervious, may retain the water at one end. Along either side the basin’s walls differ in consistency from place to place. The substratum varies in porosity, at some points being impervious like the dam, at others permitting of seepage of water to a greater or less distance from the main volume. The water continually presses against its basin walls, as if seeking to enlarge its area. And it may succeed in escaping, by slow seepage through such portions of its barrier as are pervious or soluble, or by free flow through a gap in the walls, if such offers. The area occupied by the water will extend itself most rapidly along the lines of least resistance. Every species has a center or centers of abundance in which favoring conditions usually give rise to a rate of reproduction more than sufficient to keep the critical area stocked. A tendency to occupy a larger space results, because of competition within the species: individuals and descent-lines multiply and travel radi- ally, extending those portions of the frontier where least resist- ance is offered. Such radial dispersal takes place slowly in some directions, more rapidly in others, according to the degree of passability of the opposing barriers. These barriers consist of any sort of conditions less favorable to the existence of the species than those in the center of abundance. Theoretically, sooner or later and in all directions, every species is absolutely stopped. But as a matter of undoubted fact most barriers are continually shifting, and the adaptability of the animals themselves may be also undergoing continual modification ; so that perfect adjustment is beyond the limits of possibility so long as topography and climate keep changing. The ranges of species may thus be constantly shifting. Descent- lines may move about repeatedly over the same general region, like sparks in the soot on the back of a brick fireplace. Yet, in all of our studies, of but a few years’ duration, the 248 No. 568] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 249 time element is reduced almost to a negligible quantity, and we may look upon the areas occupied by each species as, for the time of our observation, fixed. We are thus enabled to compare one with another, and because of the large number of the species, we can infer a good deal as to the nature of barriers in general, at least as regards birds and mammals. It is even conceivable that; with sufficient refinement in methods, the inquirer might in time find himself able, from a comparative study of the ranges of rodents, for example, to establish the identity of all of the external factors which have to do with the persistence of each of the species; in other words to analyze the ‘‘environmental com- plex’’ into its uttermost elements—as regards the existing species of rodents in their recent development. The most obvious kind of barrier to distribution is that con- sisting of any sort of physical, or mechanical, obstruction. Such obstruction affects directly the individuals of a species en- countering it, either by stopping their advance or by destroying outright such as attempt to cross it. As barriers of this nature, . are to be cited land in the case of purely aquatic mammals, and bodies of water to purely terrestrial, especially xerophilous, mammals. In each case the width of the barrier has to do with the degree of impassability. Oceans and continents are most perfect, and affect a large proportion of the species. The com- paratively narrow Colorado River is a barrier of the first rank, but only to a certain few desert rodents. Mechanical barriers, where they exist at all, are clearly recognizable. It is to be observed, however, upon considering the birds and mammals of a whole continent, that by far the greater number of species are delimited in range without any reference to actual land and water boundaries; more explicitly, their ranges fall far short of coast lines. The barriers here concerned are intangible, but nevertheless powerful. By their action the spread of species, genera and families is held in check as surely as by any tangible obstruction. By these invisible barriers the individual may not necessarily be stopped at all, as with animals of free locomotion; but the species is affected. For example, the mocking bird in its Cali- fornian distribution is closely confined to those parts of the state Possessing certain definite climatic features; but vagrant indi- viduals, especially in autumn, occur far beyond the limits of - these restrictive conditions. Carnivorous mammals are well 250 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII known to be subject to sporadic wanderings on the part of indi- viduals, but the species is kept in set bounds by some potent but invisible set of factors. The very fact that individuals are quite capable of temporarily transgressing these bounds and yet do not overstep them en masse emphasizes all the more the remark- able potency of this category of barriers as regards species and higher groups. Our geographic studies lead us to designate among these rela- tively intangible barriers: (1) increase or decrease in prevailing temperature beyond certain critical limits, according to the species concerned; (2) increase or decrease in prevailing atmospheric humidity beyond certain limits; (3) modification in food-supply and appropriate breeding and foraging ground. The limits set by each of these factors will vary with the physiological pecul- iarities of the organism considered; in other words the inherent structural equipment of each animal figures importantly. In these three sorts of barriers will be recognized what have been called ‘‘zonal,’’ “faunal” and ‘‘associational’’ delimitation, each of which I will now try to define. Two schools of faunistice students are represented among Amer- ican zoo-geographie writers of the present day. One, of which C. H. Merriam is the most prominent exponent, sees in tempera- ture the chief cause controlling distribution, and deals with the ranges of species in terms of ‘‘life zones.’’ The other school, of which C. C. Adams, A. G. Ruthven and Spencer Trotter are active advocates, assigns to temperature but a minor rôle, look- ing rather to a composite control, of many factors, resulting in ecologic ‘‘associations,’’ of which plants are essential elements, and which are to be further explained on historical grounds. The two sets of areas thus defined do not by any means corre- spond. Yet the reviewer can not fail to note, here and there, places where boundaries coincide, and such coincidences are so frequent as to be suggestive of real concordance in some signifi- cant manner. Is it not probable that both schools are approxi- mately correct, the difference in mode of treatment being due to different weights given the different ae of evidence, or, in other words, to difference in perspective Every animal is believed to be Sisto in distribution zonally by greater or less degree of temperature, more particularly by that of the reproductive season. When a number of animals (always in company with many plants similarly restricted) No. 568] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 251 approximately agree in such limitation they are said to occupy the same life zone. The observation of this category of distributional delimita- tion is particularly easy in an area of great altitudinal diversity like that comprised in the southwestern United States. The writer is led to wonder if those authors who minimize the impor- tance of temperature have ever been privileged to travel exten- sively, and carry on field studies, outside of the relatively uni- form eastern half of North America! Study of any area which varies widely in altitude and hence provides readily appreciable differences in daily temperature from place to place brings conviction of the very great effective- ness of temperature in delimiting the ranges of nearly all species of animals as well as of plants. Particular attention may called to the pertinent results of Merriam’s survey of Mount Shasta. ; But temperature is not to be considered the only delimiting factor of environment, though its possible overemphasis by the Merriam school seems to have led some other persons to believe that this view is held. In fact it becomes evident, after a con- sideration of appropriate data, that very many species are kept within geographic bounds in certain directions only by an in- creasing or decreasing degree of atmospheric humidity. By plotting the ranges of many animals as well as of plants coin- cidence in this regard is found in so many cases as to warrant the recognition of a number of ‘‘faunal areas’’—on the causa- tive basis of relative uniformity in humidity. It is probable that every species is affected by both orders of geographic control. The reader may enquire as to the grounds for employing the widely used terms zone and fauna in the restricted sense here prescribed. In reply, it may be said that this is not an inno- vation, but is an adoption of a usage which has come about his- torically among a certain group of workers in the geography of vertebrate animals in North America. The writer recognizes the fault in imposing restricted meanings upon old terms, but he also hesitates at coining new wo As to which is the more apòrta; assembled data seem to show that more genera and higher groups are delimited by zonal boundaries than by faunal boundaries. The arresting power of temperature barriers would therefore seem to be relatively the greater . 252 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII In the third category of distributional control there is a con- spicuous association of the majority of so-called adaptive struc- tures of animals (often of high taxonomic value) with certain mechanical, or physical, features of their environment. n animal may thus intimately depend upon certain inorganic or organic peculiarities, or both, of a given area, and be unable to maintain existence beyond the limits of occurrence of those features of the environment. Tracts of relatively uniform en- vironmental conditions, including their inanimate as well as living elements, are here called associations. After a consideration of all the birds and mammals occurring both within the state of California and elsewhere as far as the writer’s knowledge goes, associational restriction appears to be governed by the following three factors, of relative importance in the order named. 1. Kind of food-supply afforded, with regard to the inherent structural powers of each of the animals concerned to make it available. 2: Presence of safe breeding places, adapted to the varying needs of the animals, in other words depending upon the respect- ive inherent powers of construction, defence and concealment in each species concerned. 3. Presence of places of temporary refuge for individuals, during daytime or nighttime, or, while foraging, when hard- pressed by predatory enemies, again correlated with the respec- tive inherent powers of defence and concealment of each species involved It is believed that the geographical distribution of any animal is correctly diagnosed in terms of each of the three main group- ings here suggested. In other words an animal belongs simul- taneously to one or more zones, to one or more faunas, and to one or more associations. No one of these groupings can be stated in terms of the other, any more than a person can com- pute liquids by ecandle-power, or weight in miles. The constit- uent species within each of these groupings always belong to the other two. To illustrate: the southern white-headed wood- pecker inhabits the coniferous forest association of the San Bernardino fauna of the Transition zone; the Abert towhee be- longs to the mesquite and the quail-brush associations of the Colo- rado Desert fauna, of the Lower Sonoran zone; the Pacific shrew belongs to the upland riparian association of the northern coast redwood fauna of the Transition and Boreal zones. No. 568] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 253 CLASSIFICATION OF BARRIERS TO SPECIES AS REGARDS BIRDS AND MAMMALS Barriers: A. Intangible. (a’) Zonal (by temperature). (b’) Faunal (by atmospheric humidity). (c’) Associational. ) By food supply. (2) By breeding places. (3) By temporary refuges. (Each of these three with regard to the inher- ent structural characters of each species concerned.) B. Tangible (mechanical). (a”) Land to aquatic species. (b”) Bodies or streams of water to terrestrial species. The above categories are believed to include all the factors commonly involved in checking the spread of species of birds and mammals. It is possible that inter-specific competition may sometimes occur where associational homologues meet. But even here it becomes a matter of relative associational fitness which determines supremacy and consequent ultimate limits of inva- sion of the forms concerne A mountain range, sivaithiin ally speaking, is no barrier at all, per se, as frequently alleged. Only as it involves zonal or faunal barriers does it affect distribution. The same is true of a valley or a desert. As far as contemplation of cases has gone, the writer’s experi- ence has led him to believe that the outlines of the ranges of all birds and mammals may be accounted for by one or more of the factors indicated in the analysis here presented. And as de- tailed knowledge of the facts of geographical distribution accu- mulates, the delimiting factors become more and more readily detectable. By such a study, of comparative distribution, it seems possible that the ranges of birds and mammals may become Subject to satisfactory explanation. When considered in its historical bearing, the problem of barriers concerns itself intimately with the origin of species. It is believed by the writer that only through the agency of barriers Pias multiplication of species, in birds and mammals, brought about. 254 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII The present contribution is abbreviated from a general discus- sion of certain distributional problems which forms part of a paper to appear from the University of California press and which treats in detail of the birds and mammals of the lower Colorado Valley, in California and Arizona. JOSEPH GRINNELL MUSEUM OF VERTEBRATE ZOOLOGY, UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA YELLOW VARIETIES OF RATS In a recent number of the Naturauist I described a yellow variety of the common rat (Mus norvegicus) which in recent years made its appearance in England and is now a recognized variety among fanciers. Dr. John C. Phillips and Professor L. J. Cole have both called my attention to a fact which I had over- looked; namely, the occurrence of a yellow variety in another species of rat (Mus rattus). Bonhote described the occurrence of this variety in Egypt in 1910 and has since found by experiment (1912) that the yellow variation of Mus rattus is recessive in heredity precisely as it is in Mus norvegicus. The fact that the yellow variation in mice is dominant in heredity, but can not be obtained in a homozygous condition, stands, therefore, as a phe- nomenon all the more singular and striking. W. E. CASTLE. BUSSEY INSTITUTION, March 3, 1914. NOTES AND LITERATURE HEREDITY AND “THE INKLUENCE OF MONARCHS” IN ‘‘The Influence of Monarchs’’ (xiii and 422 pp., 1913, The Maemillan Co., New York, $2.00) Dr. Frederick Adams Woods makes a second and firmer step along the path entered on with his interesting ‘‘Mental and Moral Heredity in Royalty’’ published in 1906. Dr. Woods’s goal in beginning and continuing his an- alysis of the character of royalties and the circumstances of their reigns is one probably not immediately to be reached but also probably one not impossible of attainment. It is indeed not one goal that he has before him, but two, the ways to which lie close together and parallel. One is the establishing of a new science of history to be called historiometry; the other is the making ap- parent of the dominance of heredity over environment in deter- mining human fate. That the methods and even the aims of most historical study are not satisfying to all historical students is made obvious by the constant complaining of historians to and of each other. There are two conspicuous groups of these protestants, one de- manding more interest, more imagination, a more literary treat- ment of historical fact, and the other demanding a more signifi- cant, more inductive, more scientific treatment. The former wants more ‘‘humanity,’’ the latter more biology, in history. Dr. Woods is of the latter group. But Dr. Woods is not primarily of any historical camp. He is biologist, especially evolutionist and student of heredity. How- ever, he marches very boldly into the ranks of the students of his- torical human history—to distinguish thus the last few thousand years of human history from the earlier many thousand years of it—with the new methods and results of his historiometry, just as Pearson, several years ago, invaded the biological camp with his biometry. Something of historiometry in history there has always been, just as there has always been something of biometry in biology. But these reformers want to make history and biology wholly, or, at least, most importantly, sciences of measure. And each of them finds that his use of measure in them leads him to discover that the facts that he is measuring offer, in the new significance they are thus made to yield, a special argu- ment for some particular one of the major factors in evolution. 255 256 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII Biometry emphasizes the enormous importance and significance of variation in all living things; historiometry reveals the enormous importance of heredity in human life and the affairs of society. After an introductory chapter stating the need of a new inter- pretation of history and of new methods of getting at this inter- pretation, and a following general chapter further elaborating and expanding his views concerning ‘‘the philosophy of history and historiometry,’’ Dr. Woods plunges into a series of compact histories of France, Castile, Aragon, United Spain, Portugal, The Netherlands, Denmark, Sweden, Russia, Prussia, Austria, Turkey, Scotland and England. In each of these he presents a swift sum- mary of the economic and political conditions (success in wars, increase in territory and prestige, prosperity, advance, failures in war, loss of prestige, poverty, retrogression) of these nations in the various reigns of a period of about 500 years for each country, together with a statement of the personal traits of each monarch. In all, three hundred and sixty-eight monarchs, regents or other rulers, royal or non-royal, and correspondingly, three hundred _ and sixty-eight sets, or periods, of national conditions, are pre- sented. From these data is derived the very positive and important conclusion that the dominant causal influence in determining the character of national, political and economic conditions has been the personality of the monarchs, and that the prime determinant of this personality is heredity and not environment. A host of possible criticisms and objections to the method, its results and their interpretation, leaps into every one’s mind. Well, they are all—or all that I have so far been able to formu- late—anticipated, and ingeniously, and usually convincingly, answered. At least they are anticipated and discussed. In this the book reminds one of Darwin’s ‘‘ Origin of Species.”’ To all who have read ‘‘ Heredity in Royalty”? this new book of Dr. Woods will need no recommendation of its interest and im- portance. To those who have not, and are interested either as historian, biologist, or natural philosopher in human history and the bionomic factors that control it, ‘‘The Influence of Monarchs’’ may be strongly recommended as an original and very suggestive treatment of the subject. To students of heredity the book is a necessary library addition. Wi Ey Be STANFORD UNIVERSITY, CALIFORNIA VOL. XLVIII, No. 569 The American Naturalist intended for pr gece ere etc., intended for review should be MSS. sent to fo igi of THE AMERICAN rticles arashi summaries of ALIST, Garrison- -on-Hudson, New York. research work bearin on th pro seat = Fecha oer evolution are especially welcome, and wiil be given preference red reprints of sph herbie are supplied to authors free of charge. Further pimi will be supplied at c Subscription — sements should be sent to the publishers. subscription price a Nore Canadian posta a yea enty- five pei additional. The y cents and The ahaa for single copies is Foreign postage is fift forty cents. The maven ticieg rates are Four Dollars for a pa THE SCIENCE PRESS NEW TORR: : Sub-Station 84 Lancaster, Pa. A.elaad matter, April tth Garrison, N. Y. Post Office at Lancaster, Pa., under the Act ot ARE of March 3, 1879. FOR SALE ARCTIC, ee and GREENLAND BIRDS’ SKIN Well sung re Low Prices DINESEN, pok Collect Husavik, "North Iceland, Via Leidle, pR WANTED TO PURCHASE a set of BIRDS OF AMERICA by J. J. Audubon, 7 or 8 volumes, please report, stating cash price, stat- ing condition, binding and dates of volumes. F. C. HARRIS, Box 2244 Boston, Massachusetts P a TO OOLOGISTS and ENTOMOLOGISTS W. F. H. ROSENBERG Importer of Exotic Zoological Specimens 57, Haverstock Hill, London, N. W., England Begs to announce the publication of a new Price List (No. 20) of Mammals, including over 400 species from various parts of the world. will be pe free on application, as well as any ot i the following lists Birdskins (over 5000 species); Birds’ Eggs ( cee ,100 species) ; Reptiles, Batrachians and Fish lone 400 species); Exotic Lepidoptera (over 8, 000 apecies Largest stock in the world of specimens in all branches of Zoology. All Museums and Amateurs should write for these Lists All specimens sent on approval. Please state which lists are required and give Photographic Films Developed 5 cents per Spool, Prints 3 cents up, Post Cards 8x10 Enlargements 25 cents mounted. Send for Samples and Price List. FRANK R. BARBEAU 89 West Bridge Street Oswego, N. Y. a ee ee name of this periodical. eT SE The paca ad of Chicago fiers instruction during the Sum- a Quarter on the same basis he ing the t academic year. ngan colleges, the ia “ungen and the. profes kent wi fedi n given y regular members of = po ee 7 tal which is augmen n the summer by, aS Dn Summer Quarter, 1914 22 Er The Univesity of Chicago Mitchell Tower. Chicago, Minois as THE AMERICAN NATURALIST VoL. XLVIII May, 1914 No. 569 ECTOPARASITES OF MAMMALS PROFESSOR VERNON LYMAN KELLOGG STANFORD UNIVERSITY, CALIFORNIA I THE wingless permanent ectoparasites of mammals are chiefly of two groups, namely, the Mallophaga, or biting lice, which feed on the hair and dermal scales, and the Anoplura, or sucking lice, which feed on the blood. Certain mites and ticks, a few of the Pupipara (degener- ate flies) and almost all of the fleas are also ectoparasites of the mammals, but the fleas, numerous and economically important as they may be, are not permanent parasites, for they live as larve not on the host of the adult, but in cracks and crevices in floors, or in the soil and elsewhere that the organic detritus used by them as food may be found. The adults, too, hop on and off their host, and often change from one‘ host individual to another, and even from one host species to another. So that the prob- lems of distribution and species-forming with which I am particularly concerned in my studies of the ecto- parasites are not at all the same in such impermanent form as the fleas as in those truly permanent forms, the Mallophaga and Anoplura. Tn these latter there occurs an extraordinary limitation of the parasite individuals and their immediate progeny and future generations to specific and even individual hosts (and their progeny and future generations), so that the Mallophagan and Anopluran fauna of any mammal usually represents a closely inbred family strain biologically iso- 257 258 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII lated from the rest of the individuals comprising the par- ticular species represented by it. This brings about cer- tain striking conditions of abundant small variation and subspecific (or intraspecific) distinction, which, however, because of the general similarity of habitat, food and habit, do not tend to grow rapidly into large (specific, generic, family) differences. The hundred or more species of Mallophaga so far recorded from mammals have, until very recently, all been ascribed to two genera, of which one included nearly nine tenths of the total number of kinds. There has been made a beginning—and not a particularly convincing one—at breaking up this inclusive genus (Trichodectes). It is a movement suggested more by the needs of convenience than the needs of expressing a bio- logical situation. Similarly, although not representing so extreme a condition of likeness, the Anoplura, also including about a hundred parasite species (occurring only on mammals) have been, until recently, divided into but half a dozen genera, with the great majority of the species included in one. Certain aberrant forms found on man, the monkeys, the elephant, and on seals and walruses have always made necessary the recognition of four or five quite distinct genera. Attempts, however, are now being made to break up the unwieldly genus Hema- topinus. As this paper is, in effect, a continuation of my paper on ‘‘ Distribution and Species-forming of Ecto-parasites’’ published in THe Amertcan Naturauist in March, 1913, which devoted itself to a consideration of the Mallophaga (some 1,400 species as so far known) found on birds, and to the problems presented by their conditions of life and their host and geographic distribution, I can dispense with any further account of the special biology of these parasites by referring the interested reader to this former paper. In it I have set out rather fully the spe- cial structural and habit features of the Mallophaga. Except that the Anoplura take blood, rather than feathers and hair, for food, and have specially modified No. 569] ECTOPARASITES OF MAMMALS 259 mouth parts to do it with, and are perhaps even more specialized in their physiological adaptations to their host than the biting lice, most of the general remarks made concerning the Mallophaga will apply to the suck- ing lice also. In their peculiar special relations to their hosts as per- manent ectoparasites on them, wingless, and reluctant to migrate even with opportunity, and so fitted physiologi- cally to their parasitic life that they can not live for more than a few hours (or, at most, and exceptionally, days) off the bodies of their hosts, the Anoplura and Mallophaga are alike. And hence the conditions and problems of their distribution and species-forming are practically the same for the two groups. The thesis that I have maintained, on a basis of the conditions presented by the bird-infesting Mallophaga, I now wish to test by the conditions presented by the mammal-infesting Mallophagaand Anoplura. This thesis is, in fewest words, that the host distribution of these wingless permanent ectoparasites is governed more by the genetic relationships of the hosts than by their geo- graphic range, or by any other ecologic conditions. The fact, proved by abundant cases, that two host species of wholly distinct geographic range and with no possible opportunity for contact such as would permit of the migration of wingless parasites from one to the other, may have, nevertheless, one or more parasitic species common to them both, is associated almost always with the further fact that these common hosts are closely related genetically. They are most often of the same genus or of closely allied genera; they are almost cer- tainly always of the same subfamily or family. The ex- planation for the possibility and the reality of this inter- esting host distribution I find in the hypothesis that the common parasite species has persisted unchanged from a common ancestor of the now divergent but allied host kinds, Also, if it be true that genetic relationship is the deter- 260 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII mining factor in accounting for the host distribution of the parasites, then it is also true that the distribution of the parasites will indicate in some measure the genetic relationships of the hosts, and that occasional aid in determining the genetic affinities of birds and mammals of doubtful relationships may be had from a study of their parasitic fauna. In my paper already referred to I have pointed out some suggestive cases of this sort in connection with the birds and their parasites. In examining the conditions existing among the mam- mals and their Mallophagan and Anopluran fauna, the first necessity was the compilation of a complete wecord or catalogue of mammalian hosts and their parasites, together with the record of the actual locality of each finding of parasites, together with a general record of the geographic range of all the various hosts. This cata- logue, or set of records, I have now completed, and despite its meagerness compared with the similar cata- logue of the bird hosts and their Mallophagan parasites from which the notes for the former paper were drawn, it contains enough records of interest to make worth while a preliminary report on the condition obtaining among the mammals and their parasites. It is unfortunate that, although there are nearly one fourth as many mammal species as bird kinds, only about one hundred mammals figure in the Mallophagan host list, while Mallophagan parasites have been taken from over eleven hundred bird species. Also, only one hun- dred different Mallophaga have been taken from mam- mals, while about fourteen hundred have been taken from birds. Of the Anoplura, which are found only on mammals, records have been made from about one hun- dred host species, these records referring to just about the same number of Anopluran kinds. Thus the mam- malian host catalogue with its list of parasites is a short one; as far as it goes, however, it is thoroughly interest- ing and suggestive. In working up the records I have used Trouessart’s No. 569] ECTOPARASITES OF MAMMALS 261 ‘*Catalogus Mammalium’’ as an authority for the synon- omy of the hosts, and my own judgment, based on a con- siderable personal knowledge of the parasites and on a careful consideration of all the more intelligible litera- ture of the two groups, as a last court for the synonomy of the Mallophaga and Anoplura species. The synonomy of the parasites I have, however, not pushed far. With so much of introduction and explanation we may come to a swift résumé of the results of a scrutiny of these records, proceeding by sequence of the mammalian orders, and referring to either or both groups of para- sites as they may happen to be represented in the para- site records of the successive host groups. II The Marsupialia are represented in the host list by half a dozen species of kangaroos and wallabies (family Macropide) all from Australia, and a wombat, Phasco- lomys ursinus (family Phalangeride), from Tasmania (also S. Australia?). From all of these hosts only Mallo- phaga are recorded, no Anoplura having yet been taken from a marsupial. The six species of kangaroos repre- sent three genera (Macropus, Petrogale and A¢prym- nus), and their Mallophaga are of seven species, repre- senting four genera. Four of the species belong to the genus Boopia, and I strongly suspect are not all different. In addition there is one Trichodectes, from Petrogale penicillata, one Latumcephalum, from ‘‘wallabies,’’ and one Heterodoxus, which is recorded from Macropus giganteus in Australia as well as from the same host in the Jardin des Plantes, Paris. It is also recorded from an undetermined wallaby in Victoria and one in Queens- land, as well as appearing in three other records from ‘‘kangaroo”’ or ‘“‘wallaby’’ from Australia. The para- site of the wombat is a species of Boopia, and it has been twice recorded from the same host. It is interesting that the kangaroo in the Jardin des Plantes harbored, even after some period of captivity, only its own proper para- 262 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII sites without accepting new ones from its many, various and closely pressing neighbors. Of the four Mallophagan genera found on the kanga- roo, three, namely, Boopia, Latumecephalum and Hetero- doxus! are peculiar to them. The third genus, Tricho- dectes, is represented by but a single species which has been recorded but once. This is the common Mallophagan genus of mammals generally. The record is perhaps a good one, but its lack of confirmation by being unrepeated either for the same species or for any other species of Trichodectes, is suggestive. Heterodoxus, Latumcepha- lum and Boopia are two-clawed genera; that is, they are Mallophagan forms which belong to a family all the other genera of which are confined to birds. The characteristic - structural difference between the mammal-infesting Mallophaga and the bird-infesting species is the presence in the first group of a single claw on each tarsus, and in the second of two claws. This difference is plainly an adaptive one concerned with the fitting of the foot for the seizing of hairs and scrambling about among them, on the one hand, and the manipulation of feathers and moving about on them, on the other. In examining living specimens under the microscope the special use and fit- ness of the feet, in the one case adapted to hairs and in the other to feathers, is obvious. However, Heterodoxus, Latumecephalum and Boopia, and, in addition, perhaps one other doubtful genus, represented by one species, and perhaps two or three species of another two-clawed genus, constitute exceptions to the general rule. It is of decided interest to note that the only genera of two- clawed Mallophaga found exclusively on mammals are limited to the Marsupials. The antiquity and isolation of 1 The single valid species of this genus—the two or three that have been named are undoubtedly all the same—has also been recorded from dogs! In fact specimens in my own collection were received with the record ‘‘from Japanese dog.’’ And Enderlein has recorded it from a dog from China and Neumann from a dog from Formosa. Yet dogs panera do not harbor this parasite, and kangaroos and wallabies do. It seems necessary to be- lieve that the dog host records SS eases of incite from kangaroos in zoological gardens or menageri No. 569] ECTOPARASITES OF MAMMALS 263 this host group strongly suggests that the one-clawed con- dition common to all other mammal-infesting Mallophaga is a derivative from the original two-clawed condition characteristic of the parasites of birds and of these ancient mammals. The two-clawed condition is, of course, the one common to insects generally and is characteristic of the Atropids, in whom I am inclined to see the ancestors, or near-ancestors, of the Mallophaga. All of the Anoplura. it may be added, which are exclusively mammal-infesting, are one-clawed. In this connection the suggestiveness of the fact that in face of the examination of many specimens of half a dozen species of kangaroos and wallabies, no Anoplura have yet been found on the Marsupials, may be referred to. I am coming strongly to believe that there is no such wide ordinal separation of the Mallophaga and Anoplura as our clinging to the fetich of ‘‘biting and sucking mouth-parts’’ as basis for radical classificatory separation has led us to effect. I believe, with Mjöberg, that the two groups of parasites have a fairly near genealogical affinity, their differences, which are particularly those of mouth-parts, being adaptive rather than palingenetic in character. The Anoplura have gone on from the Psocid- Mallophagan condition to a more specialized parasitic habit, and are the extremes of a general line of ecto- parasitic evolution. The absence of sucking lice from the kangaroos may mean that the Marsupials are older than the Anoplura! No other considerable group of mam- mals, except certain families of strong-smelling Carni- vora, is free from the blood-sucking parasites. There are but two Edentates in the host list, one, the Cape Ant bear, Orycteropus afer (family Oryeteropo- dide) of south and central Africa, harboring a sucking louse, of genus and species peculiar to it, and the other, the three-toed sloth, Bradypus tridactylus (family Brady- podide) of eastern South America, harboring a Mallo- phagan of species peculiar to it but of the genus Gyropus which is the less scattered, although still rather catholic, 264 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII genus of the two large ones characteristic of the mammals. The large order Ungulata, with its numerous domesti- cated and semi-domesticated species, is a favorite host group with both Mallophaga and Anoplura. Altogether? about thirty Anoplura and two dozen Mallophagan spe- cies are recorded from fifty host species representing nine Ungulate families. The family Elephantide is represented by the African and Indian elephants, recognized as distinct species of distinct geographic range. They both harbor a common Anopluran species, Hematomyzus elephantis, of species, genus and family peculiar to the elephants. Fahrenholz has given the varietal name sumatranus to specimens of these sucking lice taken from an Indian elephant in Sumatra. Records show that the parasites have been taken from their elephant hosts not only in Africa and Asia, but in various zoological gardens, as Paris, Ham- burg and Rotterdam. The small family of Hyracide, or conies, is represented in the host list by two species and perhaps a third one, one of which, the Syrian coney of west and south Asia, harbors one Anopluran and one Mallophagan, while from the other, the Cape coney of South Africa, the same Anopluran species is recorded as well as another of the same genus. This record of a second species is from a coney in the London Zoological Gardens. From the pos- sible third species of Hyrax (taken in the African Congo and perhaps, but not probably, also a Cape coney), a second Mallophagan species is recorded of the same genus, Trichodectes, to which that of the Syrian coney belongs. In the family Equide three species, the horse, the donkey and Burchell’s zebra, all suffer from the infesta- tion of a common Anopluran species, Hematopinus asini. In addition, the horse and the zebra have a common 2 The synonomy in the parasite records, and indeed in the host records as well, is a vicious tangle. I have done the best I can, for the present. No. 569] ECTOPARASITES OF MAMMALS 265 Mallophagan parasite, Trichodectes parumpilosus, while the horse and donkey have another common biting louse, Trichodectes pilosus. Two varieties of Trichodectes parumpilosus have been named by Piaget, one from the zebra and another from ‘‘little horses of Java.’’ The pigs (family Suide), of which three wild African species besides the familiar animal of the barnyard are found in the host list, are infested by two (perhaps three) species of Anoplura and one (a not too certain record) Mallophagan. Hematopinus suis is found on the domes- tic Sus all over the world, while Hematopinus latus of Neumann, H. phocochoeri of Enderlein and H. peristictus of Kellogg and Paine, which are almost certainly all one species, are recorded from the wart hog, Phacocherus ethiopicus from Nyasa-land, Africa, and probably also from another wart hog species from Africa, and the Red River hog, Potamocherus cheropotamus from Nyasa- land, Africa. In addition Potamochewrus demunis (prob- ably), from German east Africa, is credited by Stobbe with a Mallophagan parasite peculiar to it, Trichodectes vosseleri Stobbe. The peccary, Dicotyles tajacu (family Dicotylide) of Central America and southwestern North America, has a Mallophagan species peculiar to it, belonging to the smaller of the two large Mallophagan genera, namely, Gyropus. The dromedary, of north Africa and western Asia, and the bactrian camel, of central Asia, harbor a common sucking louse, Hematopinus cameli. A doubtful second Species called H. tuberculatus (Neumann thinks it iden- tical with cameli) has been recorded from a dromedary imported from India into Australia. The ‘‘South Amer- ican camel,’’ the llama, harbors an Anopluran species peculiar to it, and two Mallophagan species, Trichodectes breviceps Rudow and T. inequalemaculatus Piaget. Al- though Rudow’s species are often suspect, I have just had his breviceps from a llama of Peru (collector C. H. 266 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII T. Townsend). With these llama Mallophaga there is also a small Anopluran which I have not yet worked out. The family Cervide is represented in the host list by about ten species. They are infested by three species of Anoplura, each peculiar to its host, and six species of Trichodectes (Mallophaga) of which T. tibialis is com- mon to the roe deer of Europe and Asia Minor, an African Capreolus, and our own black-tailed deer of the western states. Trichodectes longicornis is common to the red deer of Europe and Asia Minor and the fallow deer of south Europe, Asia Minor and north Africa. The giraffe (family Giraffide) harbors a sucking louse, Linognathus brevicornis, peculiar to it. The great family Bovide, with its many buffalo, buck, sheep, goat and antelope kinds, is represented in the host list by five or six species of Bos, four African bucks, three or four sheep, the ibex, chamois and two or three goats, and five or six antelopes, or gazelles. The domes- tic ox, Bos taurus, harbors three species of Anoplura and one Mallophagan. Curiously, none of these species is recorded from any other Bos. On the other hand, the zebu, the Indian buffalo, and the American bison all have the same Anopluran species (and no other, nor any Mallophagan), while the yak of central Asia and the Kaffir buffalo each have an Anopluran peculiar to it. The four species of African reedbucks and duikerboks have, according to the records, each a peculiar species of sucking louse. These records need scrutiny. One of them is my own, but I had to describe the species without seeing the types of the others. The domestic sheep carries two Anopluran species and one Mallophagan. The latter occurs also on at least two wild species of Ovis, one of west Africa and the other of north Africa. The fat-tailed sheep has a record from German south- west Africa of a Trichodectes of its own. The domestic goat harbors one Anopluran and at least one Mallophagan, the latter being common also to the Angora goat, the chamois, and a wild (?) goat of Guinea, No. 569] ECTOPARASITES OF MAMMALS 267 and a wild (?) goat of Java. A DIAGRAMMATIC DRAWING FROM A DISSECTION MADE BY TAKING A Bopy WALL OF E Fig. 1. LONGITUDINAL CUT IN THE TRAL LIN S he arrangement of the chief organs concerned in evisceration and subsequent regeneration. B, w., body wall; cl., cloaca; c. p., calcareous estine ; LITTLE T LE THE MID-VEN- canal; r, m., retractor muscles; r. t., base of respiratory tree; s., stomach; t., tentacles; m. d., mid-dorsal; 1. d., left dorsal; l. v., left ventral; r. d., right dorsal, and r. v., right ventral, interradial spaces. Fic. A DIAGRAM To SHOW THE RELATION OF RADIAL TO BILATERAL SYM- METRY. The esophagus (e) is shown in cross-section, cut just anterior to the stomach, and the view looks toward the anterior end. M., madreporite; r. ¢., ring canal, Other letters as in Fig. 1. Notwithstanding this general tendency toward bilateral symmetry, the most conspicuous differences between indi- viduals involve structures of the radial type. Fig. 1 is a diagrammatic drawing of a dissection to show the general No. 569] REGENERATION 283 arrangement of some of the more important structures studied in this experiment. The dissection was made by making a longitudinal cut in the body wall a little to the left of the mid-ventral line, and then pulling the flaps apart and pinning the animal down on its dorsal surface. The Polian vesicle is shown attached to the ring canal in the position where it is usually found when only one is present, that is in the left dorsal interradial space. It will be noticed that the retractor muscles are simply branches of the longitudinal muscles, and hence are radial in position. At the time of evisceration the body wall breaks a short distance posterior to the tentacles, the re- tractor muscles separate at the point where they join the longitudinal muscles and the intestine breaks off just in front of the cloaca. A better understanding of the radial type of structure will be gained by a reference to Fig. 2. This figure is a diagram to show the relation of the radial to the bilateral symmetry. The dorsal side of the animal is represented toward the top of the page, the esophagus appears in cross-section, cut just anterior to the stomach, and there- fore one is looking forward to the other organs shown. The retractor muscles, showing the position of the radii, are much contracted and thickened, a condition in which they are usually found after evisceration. The stone canal ending in the small madreporite is located in the mid-dorsal interradial space. Passing around in a clock- wise direction, the other interradial spaces are designated as right dorsal, right ventral, left ventral and left dorsal. Polian vesicles may be found in any of the interradii ex- cept the mid-dorsal space which always bears the stone canal. Although only one Polian vesicle is represented in this figure, the mid-ventral retractor muscle is shown double, a split condition which is characteristic when two or more Polian vesicles are present. This description will be sufficient to show the general relation between the radial and the bilateral symmetry. 284 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vow. XLVIII E\VISCERATION Only one method of producing evisceration was used. By placing a number of Thyone in a small aquarium of stagnant sea water, the supply of oxygen is soon ex- hausted. The animals become greatly distended, they crawl up on the sides of the aquarium when possible, and extend the siphon toward and frequently above the sur- face of the water. All of their behavior, including the pumping of the siphon, indicates that respiration is in- adequate. In the course of a day or two the water be- comes very foul; soon some of the Thyone will eviscerate, and a considerable percentage will do so as conditions grow more unfavorable. Many, however, resist the un- favorable surroundings and will not eviscerate though kept for several days in foul water. But if the aquarium is now placed where it will have a continuous stream of water and air bubbles passing through it, the behavior of the animals is somewhat different. They then tend to contract to a minimal size, and sometimes assume a volume not more than one fifth to one seventh of their maximum distention. The respiratory movements are practically discontinued; the animal seeks a position as close as possible to the side and bottom of the aquarium. Contraction does not always take place immediately. To my surprise, after several hours I found Thyone which had resisted the previous unfavorable conditions now dis- charging their viscera. After remaining two or three days in the running water, and the animals had appar- ently become adjusted to this condition, I again set the aquarium to one side partly filled with water. Then, by repeating the conditions of the first experiment, as the water became foul several more of the holothurians ap- parently found life too strenuous to further retain their internal organs. When the remainder of this lot of Thyone was returned to running water, and again to stagnant water, a few additional individuals underwent self-mutilation. Out of a total of sixty-one specimens used in this lot forty of them eviscerated. That is, autot- No. 569] REGENERATION 285 omy occurred in at least sixty-five per cent. of Thyone, under the conditions described. Probably one reason why this process did not occur in a still larger number is that some animals occupied more favorable positions in the aquarium. A discussion of the cause of evisceration will be given later. When evisceration occurs it is sometimes hard to see just how the process takes place. Pearse (’09) ascribes the process to a ‘‘structural accident’’; that is, it is due to a powerful contraction of the circular muscles at a time when the calcareous ring is well forward. ‘‘But if the tentacles are extended,” he says, ‘‘and the calcareous ring is pushed forward a break may occur at b” (a point in his Fig. 2 where the body wall joins the calcareous ring) ‘‘as a result of the strong contraction of the circu- lar muscles at that point, and the visceral organs are forced out. . . . Whether this autotomy takes place or not depends upon the breaking of the inner branch of the longitudinal muscle bands, whose normal function is to retract the caleareous ring. When the strain brought about by the contraction of the circular muscles becomes too great these inner bands are torn asunder, usually at the point x’’ (inner end of the retractors of the calcareous ring). While it is true that muscular contraction and consequent pressure undoubtedly plays a prominent part in the process, close observation has convinced me that this is not the only factor causing evisceration. Upon Several occasions I have watched carefully the breaking of the body wall near its attachment to the calcareous ring, and while there are times when the pressure appears to be strong, especially when the animal is being irritated mechanically, there are other times when the skin appears to ‘‘melt away’’ or separate with very little or no pres- sure present. Indeed, after the skin once breaks at one side and the viscera escape through the opening, the pres- sure is relieved. But one may observe that the skin con- tinues to break until the calcareous ring is entirely sepa- rated. This, of course, would not happen if the process 286 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vouw. XLVIII depended entirely upon an accidental structural defect. Another thing noticed is of interest in this connection. When splitting open the body wall of an animal that was eviscerating, and thus relieving any internal pressure that might be due to contraction of the circular muscles, some of the retractors were seen still attached to the longitu- dinal muscles. Under these conditions it would not be possible for the retractors to exert any pull against the pressure produced by the circular muscles, yet the re- tractors were observed to constrict off or break away from the longitudinal muscles by what appeared to be purely a local disturbance. It is hard to see how this could happen, or how the skin continues to separate around the calcareous ring after the first break is made, if the process of evisceration depends solely upon the breaking of retractors and internal pressure. Indeed, the view that local changes take place in the tissues is supported by other facts. Leptosynapta, if left in stag- nant water or under other favorable conditions, under- goes repeated autotomous fission as the result of local constrictions, and Pearse states that autotomy depends upon the presence of the anterior portion of the body, and presumably upon the presence of the circumoral nerve ring. However, he found in Thyone that highly irritating substances like acetic acid and clove oil did not produce ejection of the viscera. Nor were drugs like codene and atropine, which cause violent peri- staltie waves of contraction to pass over the body, any more potent in in- ducing autotomy. The same may be said of sodium chloride, atropine and clove oil, although the injection of any of these substances was often followed by a waving of the oral tentacles to perform feeding movements, thus bringing about favorable anatomical relations for au- totomy. These results would indicate that the nervous system is not primarily involved. Certainly the ejection of vis- cera may occur in Thyone without cord visible external stimulus. The parts eviscerated in Thyone have already been No. 569] REGENERATION 287 mentioned. However, sometimes evisceration is incom- plete, as the following examples will show. On the morn- ing of August 4, a Thyone, which we shall later speak of as individual H, was found eviscerating in an over- crowded aquarium jar. While the process usually re- quires only a few seconds, or at most a few minutes, the intestine in this case was not completely thrown out until two or three hours later. This animal lived until killed at the end of twenty-one days. In the afternoon of the same day on which individual H eviscerated, an- other Thyone was found with the process only partially complete. Five hours later the intestine was still re- tained, and scissors were used to cut it off at its anterior end near the stomach. Though this Thyone received equally good care it died at the end of two days without further evisceration. A third specimen was found in- completely eviscerated on the above date, but it was allowed to stand until the next morning; at this time the injured end was open, the intestine was still within the body cavity and a part of one of the branchial trees was protruding. The intestine was pulled out and broken off, after which the branchial tree was retracted and the injured end partially closed. This animal also died at the end of two days. A fourth Thyone was seized and by squeezing was forcibly caused to throw off the usual parts except the following: a part of the stomach, most of the intestines, and some of the retractor muscles which had broken off near their esophageal end. The next morning it had expelled the remainder of the stomach and intestine, two complete retractor muscles, and some débris which had escaped from the intestine into the body cavity. The anterior end of the part re- maining appeared ragged and imperfectly closed. It died on the third day. It is probable that the two re- tractor muscles last expelled were broken off at their posterior ends by local constriction, not when the body was under pressure. A fifth animal, which we shall designate as individual M, was found partly eviscerated 288 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVII late on the afternoon of August 6. The next day it still retained the stomach and intestine and at noon the diges- tive tube was clipped off with scissors in the region of the esophagus. Nothing peculiar was noted in its behavior until four days later, August 11, when it discharged the remainder of the digestive tube. It lived and was killed at the end of eighteen days. These results are typical. The animal dies unless it is itself able to eliminate all organs concerned in the process of evisceration, and therefore regeneration does not occur unless all these organs are eliminated. The eviscerated animals show comparatively a low degree of mortality. In an attempt to raise twenty-five mutilated Thyone seven died; three of these were un- able to complete the process of evisceration as described above, and two more, since they lived for fourteen days, probably owe their death to other causes. The sixth specimen to die lived three days and had been slow in eviscerating. The seventh did not receive the best of care and died after three days. So considering the amount of injury the mortality is extremely small where proper care is taken and evisceration is complete. It will not be inopportune to describe the subsequent behavior of the different parts after evisceration. The parts expelled lie on the bottom in a more or less inactive condition until they die, which happens usually in the course of a few hours. At first the tentacles frequently expand and contract. They are highly sensitive, as one would expect, and if touched withdraw quickly into the esophagus and at the same time the retractor muscles will undergo strong contraction. By supporting these parts near the surface of the water, so as to insure plenty of oxygen, an attempt was made to keep them alive. In some cases the parts remained alive for two or three days, so this experiment appeared to be partially success- ful. Death is probably due to the direct exposure of tissues to the sea water and to the attacks of minute organisms. The dermo-muscular portion of Thyone is No. 569] REGENERATION 289 much less sensitive than the expelled portion, just after evisceration. This is due to lack of a central nervous system. Benavior Durinc REGENERATION After evisceration each specimen was placed in a sepa- rate jar of fresh sea water. The injured end of the body turns in and closes up tightly, and the entire body is somewhat smaller than before evisceration. Respira- tion is slower and not so vigorous. If the water is stag- nant, within a few hours the animal usually climbs up on the side of the aquarium by means of its tube feet. This part of the animal therefore is capable of respond- ing to a lack of oxygen, and the reaction is independent of the central nervous system. The observations upon the following individual, re- ferred to in my notes as Thyone A, will serve to illus- trate the general behavior during regeneration: July 14, A.m.—Animal eviscerated itself in the usual way. In the afternoon it climbed up on the side of the jar and clung there evidently for the purpose of respiration. July 15-16.—Acts as on the afternoon of the fourteenth. Keeps closed and well contracted at the injured end. Entire body somewhat smaller than before evisceration, due in part to organs lost. Respira- tion slower and not so vigorous as norm July 17.—In the afternoon, after watar was changed, Thyone took up position on the sand against the side of the jar farthest away from the source of light. July 18.—The next morning it was half buried in the sand in same Tr, with a few pieces of débris pulled over it. Remained so all ay. July 23.—For some two days it has been slowly burrowing down until only the two protruding ends of the body can be seen. When a piece of débris that was being held over a part of the anterior end was touched, this end retracted below the surface and the posterior end withdrew until it could scarcely be seen. Later the posterior end re- tracted when the shadow of my hand passed over it, the hand being held about one foot away. The uninjured animal is even more sensitive to shadow. The respiratory movements are growing stronger. July 28.—For the past two or three days the Thyone has been slowly moving through the sand in a posterior direction without uncovering itself, 290 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII August 2.—It is now oriented with respect to the direction of the light and has reached probably the darkest portion of the jar. August 7.—Has advanced still farther. Came about half way out of the sand to do this. August 8.—Reacts quickly to shadows by iar inlet and to jar- ring tha ak Evidently is recovering its normal behav August 10.—Has again come up about half way out a the sand. Reacts quickly to shadows as before. ugust 11.—Came entirely out of the sand. Spent the day on the sand or on the side of the jar. Appeared restless. August 12, 4 p.m.—Has been clinging to the side of the jar and mov- ing about more or less all day. Respiratory movements are strong and apparently normal. Has just now expanded the anterior end suffi- ciently for me to see the new growth of tissue formed around a penta- gonal opening. Fifteen minutes later it was observed to extend a set of minute tentacles and go through feeding movements. The tentacles ap- peared to be slightly more than three eighths of an inch in length. Its behavior continued apparently normal until it was killed twelve days later. The actions of other Thyone were studied under the same conditions, and we shall now give a general sum- mary of their behavior during regeneration. The earli- est reactions after evisceration take the form of contrac- tions resulting in the closure of the wound, and move- ments in response to lack of oxygen. If the oxygen supply is sufficient Thyone will draw itself closely into the angle between the side and bottom of the aquarium, or if the supply is deficient, it clings close to the side of the jar near the surface. In from three to seven days an instinct to burrow usually asserts itself. There is a tendency for the body to contract very noticeably at this time, and the whole organism becomes rather inactive. This condition is probably necessary for the formation of new tissue. Pearse makes the statement that in bur- rowing the normal Thyone will cover itself in from two to four hours. My observations on the mutilated ani- mals indicate that they require from twelve to twenty- four hours, in one case forty-eight hours, to complete the reaction. The process frequently stops for some hours and occasionally is never completed. In the Thyone de- No. 569] REGENERATION 291 scribed above the animal did not begin to orient itself with respect to the source of light until about the twelfth day, but in another case the response took place on the second day, which shows that this reaction does not de- pend upon the central nervous system. It should be stated that normal Thyone similarly placed were used as controls. Thyone A was quite sensitive to shadows and to touch on the ninth day, but it reacted more quickly on the twenty-fourth day both to shadows and to mechanical disturbances. Whether this was due to the regeneration of a new central nervous system, or to a more highly developed specialization of function in the old tissue, I am unable to say. It is quite possible that both factors were involved. Respiration is undoubtedly correlated with the activity of the animal, and feeding movements do not occur until the regeneration of all organs is well established, at about twenty-seven or twenty-eight days. The internal changes that take place during regenera- tion were studied in animals that were killed at different stages in the process. Thyone N was killed nine days after self mutilation. At the injured end there was a very small plug of tissue representing the newly formed esophagus; a thread-like continuation of this tissue, the beginning of a new stomach-intestine, was also seen in the mesentery. The calcareous ring and the ring canal were not clearly defined. Another Thyone was killed at about the same age after evisceration; India ink was in- jected into the cloaca and into the opening at the ante- rior end in an attempt to demonstrate a cavity in the newly formed thread-like, stomach-intestine. The re- sults were negative and the esophagus was found to be tightly closed. However, the interesting observation was made that the anterior end of each of the longitudinal muscles had split off a very slender branch to form a new retractor muscle (see Fig. 3). These newly formed retractor muscles were not more than one fourth inch in length; their anterior ends were attached in a normal Position around the esophagus, but their posterior ends 292 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII were attached only a short way back, much in front of the position of attachment of the full-sized retractors. In another animal killed when a day or so older, the same conditions held with reference to esophagus, stomach and intestine. At least three of the radial canals belong- ing to the water vascular system had branched and con- nected at their anterior ends in such a manner as to form a part of a new ring canal (cf. Fig. 4). I was un- able to find the rest of the ring-canal and perhaps it was not yet mea Fig. 3. Fic. Fic, 3. DIAGRAMMATIC DRAWING TO SHOW THAT IN Ro oN. THE RE- hera MUSCLES (r. m.) ARISE BY SPLITTING OFF FROM THE LONGITUDINAL MUSCLE .m.). Dissected a little to the right of the mid-ventral line; d., dor- sal squall ae suspending the intestine (i.); in., integument; e., region of og Fie. O SHOW THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE PATUN CANAL IN A THYONE Kobi aa OR oe DAYS AFTER EVISCERATION, radial canal; p., pentagonal The terior ends of the radial canals ak neisti and these vegn pe erR ose to form the canal which later assumes a circular shape around the esophagus. Thyone F, which was killed twelve days after eviscera- tion, showed minute calcareous plates which formed a very small esophageal ring not more than one millimeter in diameter. The esophagus continued posteriorly in the form of a small tube, the stomach-intestine, which was suspended in the dorsal mesentery. This new digestive tube was about 0.5 millimeter in diameter and contained small, colored, movable particles that could be seen with the unaided eye. The ring canal was completely formed. Another specimen, Thyone O, died at the end of four- No. 569] REGENERATION 293 teen days and was in bad condition when examined. The stomach had begun to expand and retractor muscles were present. Probably owing to the condition of the speci- men, no calcareous ring, ring canal, or Polian vesicle could be found. Another individual killed at about fif- teen days showed the stomach slightly enlarged, and the intestine, retractor muscles, calcareous ring, tentacular canals, and ring canal well formed. Two small Polian vescicles each about one millimeter in length were pres- ent. The position of the new intestine was described in my notes as follows: From the stomach the intestine follows the ventral edge of the dorsal mesentery, lying ventral to the gonaduct. At the gonad it turned ven- trally with the mesentery and then forward for about one half inch to the left interradial space; here it turns rather abruptly backward, con- tinuing in the mesentery below the left branchial tree to the anterior ventral part of the cloaca. At a little later stage in another specimen the intestine passed from the left ventral interradial to the right ven- tral interradial space; then posteriorly and again to the left, following the ventral radial mesentery to the ante- rior ventral side of the cloaca. We see from the preceding description that all impor- tant organs have been reproduced in form though not in size, before the end of the fifteenth day. The first madre- porite with its tiny stone canal was found some eighteen days after mutilation. Twenty-one days after eviscera- tion in one specimen the caleareous ring was about three millimeters in diameter and the ampulle at the bases of the tentacles were well developed. Within a week after this time the regenerating animal begins active feeding. Thyone A, killed at 41 days, was practically a normal animal both in behavior and appearance, except for the fact that the regenerated organs had not yet reached full size. The stomach was about one third normal size, but the Polian vescicles were better developed. The intes- tine contained a small amount of food material and was nine or ten inches in length; most of this growth had 294 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vor. XLVIII taken place posterior to the gonad. It was held in posi- tion as previously described and had several additional coils. [INDIVIDUAL VARIATIONS To all outward appearances any two Thyone are as much alike as two peas. It was not until the internal organs were studied that important differences were ob- Fie. 5. see TO SHOW VARIATION IN POSITION AND SIZE OF THE POLIAN VESICLES, P. v., Polian vesicles; m., madreporite; r. c., ring canal; a-d, with one Polian v Manila a e-g, with two; h-k, with three, 1l., with four; c., d., f., 9. E- with additional rudiments of these vesicles; j., with a branched vesicle. served. While there are numerous minor differences, the most conspicuous variations are found in the num- ber, size and location of the Polian vesicles (cf. Fig. 5), and in the number and arrangement of the retractor muscles. On account of the radial structure of Thyone not more than four Polian vesicles are present, since No. 569] REGENERATION 295 a homologous structure, the madreporite and its stone canal, occupies the dorsal interradial space. The num- ber of vesicles varies in fact from one to four. By a reference to Table I, it will be seen that out of 77 indi- vidually examined, 41 had one, 20 had two, 14 had three, TABLE I To SHOW THE NUMBER OF POLIAN VESICLES PRESENT IN A GIVEN NUMBER OF THYONE. ALSO TO SHOW THEIR LOCATION IN THE INTERRADIAL SPACES, WITH REFERENCE TO THE BILATERAL SYMMETRY OF THE ANIMAL Number of Number of Individ- Left | Left | Right Right Polian Vesicles uals Examined Dorsal | Ventral | Ventral Dorsal 1 41 les a: 0 2 20 Wooa o d 3 1 3 14 14 | 15 | 12 1 4 2 2o] 2o 2 2 TOM ee ee. Ta ee ee R 4 and 2 had four Polian bodies. If one is to test the matter of regeneration, of course it is important to know whether the variations or individual peculiarities will be accurately reproduced. Another striking character- istic comes out when we note in the same table the loca- tion of these organs. Of the forty-one individuals which had a single Polian vesicle, all were on the left side of the animal, and 38 were in the left dorsal interradial space. In twenty specimens with two Polian bodies each, 36 were on the left side and only four on the right side of the body. A similar asymmetrical distribution of these parts was found when three Polian bodies were present. In one specimen, however, two vescicles were found in one space, the left ventral interradius, the only instance of this kind observed; on account of this dou- bling, the right side lacked one of the number to which it was entitled in the table. Where four Polian bodies are present the arrangement is, of course, symmetrical on both sides. Still another interesting fact comes out when we examine the totals in the last line. Out of the 77 indi- viduals, 71 had a Polian vesicle in the left dorsal inter- radial space, 39 vesicles were found in the left ventral, 296 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII 17 in the right ventral, and only 4 in the right dorsal space. That is, the total number on the left side com- pared with the total number on the right side bears the ratio of 110 to 21.. Not only is there this tendency for the vesicles to be more abundant on the left side of Thyone, but the totals show that the chances of a given Thyone having a Polian vesicle in any given interradial space decreases in a counter-clockwise direction, begin- ning with the left dorsal interradial position. Coincid- ing with the number of individuals examined, the maxi- mum number of chances is found in the mid-dorsal inter- radius, where the stone canal is always present. That is, the stone canal with its madreporite is a more funda- mental and stable structure than each or all of the vesicles. The conditions are none the less interesting when we compare the Polian vesicles with reference to size and location, as will be seen from the examination of Table II. The Polian vesicles are here divided arbitrarily into three groups, designated as large, medium and small, and their respective locations are shown. In addi- TABLE II To SHOW THE POLIAN VESICLES WITH REFERENCE TO SIZE AND LOCATION Size | Left Dorsal Left Ventral | Right Ventral Right Dorsal Total | | | Largo. i... | 56 17 0 | 0 73 Medium..... | 17 22 5 | 1 45 Smal.. | 0 0 10 | 3 13 Rudiment... | 2 1 5 | 7 15 Toa... | 75 o a 20 | 11 146 tion some Thyone had the rudiments of other vescicles, each too small to be considered a distinct pouch. These are designated in the table as a ‘‘rudiment.’’ It will be noticed that all of the large, and most of the medium- sized vesicles are on the left side; that all the small ones, and most of the rudimentary ones are on the right side. The table as a whole shows that not only does the number of Polian vesicles diminish in a counter-clockwise No. 569] REGENERATION 297 direction, but their size diminishes following the same law. These facts appear significant and without doubt are suggestive of ancestral history. If it is true that the radial symmetry of Echinoderms is to be ascribed to a fixed stage in their ancestral his- tory, we are led to suppose that the point of attachment was on the right side of an originally bilateral animal. The life history of Pentacrinus, the larval organ of Aste- roidea, and a great many anatomical and embryological facts support this view. While it is not within the prov- ince of this paper to discuss the relative significance of these matters, the evidence is so overwhelming that the theory is generally accepted. It is also no doubt true that some groups of Echinoderms took to a free-living existence early in their ancestral history, and others re- mained fixed until comparatively a late period. As proof we may cite the embryological evidence that Holothurians develop without any attached stage whatever, that the Asteroids develop a larval organ and pass through a- Sessile stage for a brief period in their development, while the crinoids usually remain permanently fixed throughout life. At least we can best account on this theory for the deep-seated and fundamental radial sym- metry of some forms; the longer the attachment the more deep-seated would become the type of radial sym- metry. Now if this theory is correct we can use it to ex- plain the conditions described above for Thyone. The ancestors of this form must have broken away from the fixed stage very early, for we find the radial symmetry not well established on the right side of the animal as evidenced by both the position and size of the Polian vesicles. Out of 118 large and medium-sized Polian vesicles, 112 were on the left side, while in a total of 28 small or rudimentary Polian bodies, 25 were found on the right side. The arrangement of these organs in Thyone adds one more bit of evidence to support the following statement of Lankester. 298 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII It therefore appears that the Holothurian stock branched off from the Pelmatozoa before complete pentamerous symmetry of the hydro- coele and associated organs had arisen, before any definite caleynal sys- tem had developed, while the gonads were still a simple strand opening to the exterior by a single posterior gonopore. The muscles used as retractors of the esophagus were other organs in which there was considerable individual variation. As a general rule each of the five retractor muscles consists of a single band that takes its origin from the longitudinal radial muscle about one third the way back from the anterior end of the body and is inserted in front into the wall of the esophageal ring. Such a retractor, however, is frequently split up into several strands vary- ing from two to five in number. A reference to Table IIT TABLE III To SHOW THE CORRELATION BETWEEN THE NUMBER OF POLIAN baar AND THE TENDENCY FOR THE RETRACTOR MUSCLES TO DIVID Number of Polian Vesicles [24 es soj a | | Retractor muscles; single.) es a ey et | 39 2 0 0 Retractor muscles, multiple. .................... ee. |17 5 2 Average n aeres retractor muscles, per individual. . .| 5.153 10.263) 12.400, 10-000 Average number retractor muscles, per radius. ..... eet 030) 2.06211 2: 480) 2 2.000 shows that in 76 individuals examined, 41 had retractor muscles all in single bands, while 35 specimens had these muscles subdivided or multiple in character. This vari- ation is especially interesting when considered with reference to the number of Polian vesicles. For in forty cases where one Polian body was present thirty-nine bore the unsplit or single retractor and there was only one specimen with these muscles showing a multiple number. In thirty-six cases where two or more Polian vesicles were present, all but two had the retractor muscles in a split or divided condition. If we consider each strand as a separate retractor muscle, we may then obtain the average number of retractors per individual for any definite number of Polian vesicles. By a reference to the fourth horizontal line of Table III, one finds that the average number in individuals with one Polian vesicle is No. 569] REGENERATION 299 just slightly in excess of five, the pentameric number, and the average number when two Polian vesicles are present is 10.263. This ratio is only partly maintained when three vesicles are present, for the average number is then 12.400, and in the two cases with four vesicles the average was just twice the pentameric number. It is therefore evident from the facts shown in this table that with an increase in the number of Polian vesicles there is associated a strong tendency for the retractor muscles to take on a split character. If it were not for the fact that the split character shows considerable variation in the same individual one might suggest that the tendency to divide is correlated with the greater functional activ- ity of the water vascular system as evidenced by the in- creased number of Polian vesicles and the location of the longitudinal muscles that lie along and just internal to the radial canals. About all one can say is that corre- lated with a more complete radial symmetry with respect to the Polian vesicles, there is a greater plasticity in the retractor muscles, causing them to divide longitudinally into separate muscle bands. To what extent, or how completely, may these indi- vidual variations be reproduced in the process of regenera- tion? An answer was obtained in the following way. First a close examination was made of all parts eviscer- ated and a record was kept of all organs showing variable structures. Special attention was given to Polian vesicles and to retracter muscles. The mutilated specimens were then placed in separate aquaria in which the water was changed frequently to prevent it from becoming stale. After a considerable interval these animals were killed and the regenerated organs were compared with the lost parts. Table IV shows several individuals compared in this way. The number of retractor muscles found in each radius is given in the order of the radii taken in a clock- wise direction. A study of the table indicates that there is a strong tetndency to reproduce individual peculiarities, as shown by individuals B, E, G, H, M and O. This does 300 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII not always hold true, for individual L reverted toward the more radial type of symmetry. From these few cases it would appear that individual peculiarities tend to pre- dominate over ancestral influences in the process of re- TABLE IV To ILLUSTRATE THE RELATION BETWEEN REGENERATION AND ORIGINAL SYM- METRY IN THYONE Tnaividuai Original Symmetry Regenerated Symmetry Uses Polian Vesicles Retractor Muscles | Polian Vesicles Retractor Muscles B 2-—2-—2-—2-2 2 3-3-—2-3-3 E 2+ 3-3-2-2-2 2 2-83-2-2-2 G 1-1-1-1-1 2 1-1-1 A 2 1—2-—2-—2-1 2+ 2-2-2-—2— L 1 1-1-1-1-1 2 2-3-2-2-2 M 1 1-1-1-1-1 7 1—2-1-1-2 O 3+ | 2-—2-2-2-2 ? 2-2-1-2-2 WwW Bh AA 2 2-2—2—3—4 > OE Aree E ad SE 2 2-2—2—2-2 Ee ea a a E a eo 2 2—2—1-—2-2 generation. Specimens W, X, Y, are included in this table to show further the correlation between Polian ves- icles and retractor muscles. Discussion AND SUMMARY There remains to be discussed the general bearing of the foregoing experiments. First, the difference in the number of Polian vesicles in different Thyone is partly compensated by a variation in size, the fewer the number the larger their size, though this ratio would not be an exact one. In other words the total volume of the Polian vesicles in any given specimen bears a general relation to the size and functional activity of the animal. Notwith- standing this functional relationship since the actual number varies so widely it would be interesting to com- pare the number found in other species of holothuria with the conditions in Thyone. The data secured on this ques- tion were meager and not very definite. For example, Packard in one of the older text-books says in speaking of Thyone, No. 569] REGENERATION 301 There are three Polian vesicles, one fusiform and an inch in length, the two others slenderer. Clark (’02) gives the number for Thyone briareus (Leseur) as usually one or two; for T. scabra (Verrill) as usually single, and for T. wnisemita (Stimpson) as one. He also mentions six other holothurians found in the Woods Hole region and all have a single Polian ves- icle except Cucumaria frondosa (Gunnerus), which usu- ally has one. He says nothing of the position in which these vesicles are found. In another paper (’01) Clark mentions a large holothurian about 40-45 centimeters in length (Holothuria mexicana Ludwig) in which there is a great diversity in the number of tentacles and Polian vesicles. The tentacles vary from 18 to 21, while the Polian vesicles vary from 1 to 9. The number of speci- mens examined, sixteen, was hardly sufficient to obtain an adequate comparison; two had 1 vesicle each, two had 2, five had 3, three had 7, one had 8, and one had 9. It is probable that if one were to examine a large number of individuals of each species, with reference to the number and location of the vesicles, he would obtain further inter- esting results. Lang (’96) cites a number of groups of holothurians in which only one vesicle has been observed; but states that there are a number of species in other groups that have occasionally or usually more than one. Where accessory vesicles oceur they vary greatly in number, and ap- pear to have very slight, if any, systematic significance. Where only one Polian vesicle occurs it lies in the left ventral interradius, very seldom in the left dorsal interradius. Where two or more vesicles occur, they are also mostly formed in the ventral region of the cireular canal. Since Lang describes Cucumaria as the type specimen, in which the Polian vesicle is said to be in the left ventral region, it is possible that his generalizations were based principally on this form. At any rate, the conditions in Thyone seem to give a more definite significance to the number and location of the Polian vesicles. Various explanations of autotomy and evisceration have been suggested, many of them having a teleological char- acter. The view that the holothurian offers up the better 302 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vow. XLVIII part of itself to appease the hunger of its enemy lacks confirmation, since the viscera are distasteful to fishes and to some other animals. It may be that the autotom- ous elimination of the Cuvierian organs serves a defen- sive purpose, as pointed out by Ludwig and Minchin, and Minchin suggests that the viscera may also be lost in this process and thus incidentally be associated with a pro- tective response. In the case of Thyone, however, evis- ceration can hardly be considered defensive, and certainly it is not a process of self-division for only one part pro- duces a new individual. Clark (’99) in discussing self- mutilation in the synaptas states the matter clearly in the following terms: I agree entirely with Cuenot (’91) in believing that autotomy is not normal or defensive but is due entirely to pathological conditions. I never saw a case of it in synaptas supplied with plenty of sand and an abundance of sea water. Lang (’96) points out one of these pathological condi- tions, and recounts the fact that A Stichopus was observed to come entirely out of its skin, i. e., the whole integument dissolved into slime, so that only the dermo-muscular tube enclosing the viscera remained. In the present paper I have mentioned that Thyone at times appears to undergo a similar softening of the tissues in the region where the break occurs, and Pearse (’09) showed that autotomy is due, at least in part, to a structural arrangement which he considers is accidental in character. My observations further show that local constrictions undoubtedly have an important part in sepa- rating the retractors from the radial longitudinal muscles. All of these factors are pathological and are due to exter- nal or internal stimuli. The external (extra-cellular) stimuli, mechanical and chemical, as tried by Pearse (’08), appear to be less effective in producing autotomy than the purely internal (intracellular) stimuli such as lack of oxygen and its associated phenomena. The chem- ical (strychnine) that produced the largest percentage of evisceration in Pearse’s experiments, probably affected respiration, since it greatly increased the activity of the No. 569] REGENERATION 303 animal; therefore the need of oxygen would be propor- tionately greater than the supply, and the Thyone ren- dered more susceptible to evisceration. Now while autot- omy undoubtedly enables the animal to maintain its exist- ence for a considerable period on a smaller supply of oxygen, the times when this would become necessary in nature are probably rare, and it would be futile to specu- late upon what evolution yet has in store for the process. According to Lang, the retractor muscles of the oral region have been derived by the splitting up of the ori- ginally simple longitudinal muscles, and this specializa- tion became more marked as the oral tentacles became more highly developed and required increasing protec- tion. Species are to be found in the Dendrochirote in which the separation and branching off of retractors from the longitudinal muscles has not yet been perfected. In regeneration the retractor muscles of Thyone are derived in the same way, i. e., by splitting off from the longitu- dinal muscles, and such progress is made that they are fairly well developed by the time the tentacles take up the function of feeding. The increasing sensitiveness and the later activity of the regenerating animal are presumably associated with the development of a new nervous system. If we may regard the bilateral echinoderm larva as representing an early phylogenetic stage rather than a larval adaptation to a free-swimming existence, we will now discuss the symmetry of Thyone. As stated above, it is generally agreed that the radial arrangement of parts of the echinoderm body is due to a fixed stage in its ancestral history. Some holothurians and spantangoids, show in their ontogeny first a free stage, second a radial stage, and finally a bilateral adult. During the develop- ment of asteroids that have a fixed embryonic stage, the early bilateral symmetry is soon disarranged by the development of organs on the left side of the animal. For example, the left hydrocæle takes the form of an un- closed water-vascular rosette which grows around the esophagus to form the ring canal and its appendages, and its connection with the dorsal pore gives rise to the stone 304 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII canal. Excepting the echinoids and crinoids in which there is either no distinct Polian vesicle or else a simple glandular structure, those echinoderms that have retained the most distinctive type of radial structures have also as a rule, retained the most symmetrical arrangement of the Polian vesicles. Presumably these forms, the aster- oids and ophiuroids, have quite recently abandoned the fixed stage, and each individual usually has four Polian vesicles and a stone canal, one in each interradius. Among most of the holothurians a secondary bilateral symmetry has become superimposed over the radial type, and it is reasonable to suppose that there was a time in the ancestral history of Thyone when the Polian vesicles were symmetrically and radially disposed, or else the animal quit its fixed habits before the radial symmetry of the vesicles was thoroughly established. In the one case we would have a regression, a sort of backward retracing of the steps of evolution, or, which seems more probable, the ancestors of Thyone began a free-living existence before the radial arrangement of the Polian vesicles had become complete. Also the fact that the embryology of the holothurian egg is probably much compressed and shows no trace of a fixed stage indicates that the corresponding ancestral stage was compara- tively short, or, very remote. Since the modern habits of Thyone are bilateral, and since it is altogether improb- able that such habits would produce the present arrange- ment of Polian vesicles, the position of these organs must be due to ancestral influence. Now the Polian vesicles are capable of contracting and expanding and their function when they are well devel- oped is to act as accessory reservoirs of the water-vas- cular fluid. Muscle and connective tissue in the wall of the vesicle furnish the means to do this work. Of course, if the ampulle are well developed there is little or no need of Polian vesicles, as is the case in Asterias. But, though the size and number of these vesicles is function- ally correlated with the general development of the water-vascular system, especially of the oral tentacles, ` No. 569] REGENERATION 305 and hence shows great variability in the different species of holothurians, this does not in any way explain the great excess of these vesicles on the left side of Thyone briareus. In regeneration, probably through the influence of functional correlation, there is a tendency for the old tissue to reproduce the exact number and arrangement of the lost vesicles, but it may reproduce a somewhat more radial (ancestral) arrangement. Enough has been given in this paper to show the need of a more extensive and intensive reexamination of the Polian vesicles. This would give a better idea of their morphological and functional significance. The follow- ing summary and conclusions are based on the work described: 1. Evisceration in Thyone includes the following or- ` gans: Esophagus, stomach, intestine, calcareous ring, nerve ring, tentacles, ring canal, Polian vesicles, stone canal with madreporite, and the retractor muscles of the esophagus. 2. The method used to produce evisceration was to allow Thyone to stand in stagnant water until it became foul. This was followed by treatment with running water containing much oxygen. Alternating these processes produced as high as 65 per cent. of self-mutilated indi- viduals. 3. The structural accident theory of Pearse is inade- quate to explain all of the conditions arising in the proc- ess of autotomy. At times the skin appears to dissolve away with little or no pressure present, and retractors frequently break off by local constrictions instead of by longitudinal pull. 4. The parts eviscerated are at first highly irritable, and may be kept alive for some time. The part remain- ing is less responsive, but reacts to touch, to lack of oxygen, and probably to other stimuli. 5. Regeneration of all lost organs may occur, but it takes place only when all parts concerned in evisceration are completely expelled. Otherwise the animal dies. 306 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [VoL. XLVIII 6. During the process of regeneration the behavior gradually becomes more responsive and finally is like the normal individual. This appears to be correlated with the growth of a new nervous system. 7. Thyone is functionally a bilateral animal, but the most conspicuous individual differences involve struc- tures that have.a radial arrangement. 8. The Polian vesicles vary greatly in number, size and location. There is a strong tendency for these to occur on the left side, and this arrangement is undoubt- edly due to ancestral conditions, for the present bilateral habits of Thyone could probably have no influence in producing this asymmetry. 9. The retractor muscles in a single radius consist of single or multiple strands, and this variation is closely correlated with a similar variation in the number of Polian vesicles. No explanation is forthcoming for this peculiar plasticity of the retractor muscles, but the sug- gestion is made that it may be functionally correlated with the development of the water-vascular system. 10. It was found from the study of a number of speci- mens that individual peculiarities of structure tend to be reproduced in the process of regeneration. In this proc- ess it would appear that individual variations tend to predominate over generalized ancestral influence. 11. Autotomy enables Thyone to survive for a consid- erable period on a smaller than normal supply of oxygen. Nevertheless, the conditions which give rise to self-muti- lation are seemingly in all cases pathological. 12. The conditions in Thyone afford some evidence for believing that when this animal abandoned the fixed stage the Polian vesicles conformed more or less to the radial type. This is opposed to the statement of Lang that in all cases where a multiple number is now present ‘‘there was originally only one vesicle.” It is believed that the present arrangement of Polian vesicles in Thyone can be best accounted for on the theory of phylogenetic influ- ence. That, in general, those vesicles have retained their most complete radial arrangement in those species of No. 569] REGENERATION 307 echinoderms which have maintained to a high: degree the functional activity of the water-vascular system. REFERENCES Bather, F. A., and Goodrich, E. S. 3 A Treatise on Zaldi. Ed. by E. Ray Lankester. Pt. 3. The Echinodermata. London Clark, A. H. 09. The Affinities of the Echinoidea. Am. Nart., Vol. XLIII, pp. 682-686, Clark, H. L. 79 The Synaptas of the New England Coast. Bull. U. S. Fish Com., 1899, pp. 21-31. 700. The peng ey of Porto Rico. Bull. U. S. Fish Com., 1900, PpP. —263. 02. The Tandon of the Woods Hole Region. Bull. U. S. Fish Com., 1902. Cuenot. 9] rongy morphologiques sur les Echinodermes. Archiv. de Biol., PAR Gerould, J. hs 96. Anatomy and Histology of Caudina arenata. Proc. Boston Soc. at, Hist., Vol. 27, pp. 8-74. Grave, C, 03. On the Occurrence among Echinoderms of Larve with Cilia ar- ranged in Transverse Rings, with a Suggestion as to their Sig- nificance. Biol. Bull., Vol. V, pp. 169-186. 05. The Tentacle Reflex in a ee tk Cucumaria pulcherima. Johns Hopkins Univ. Cire., Vol. 24, pp. 504-50 Henri, V. 03. Etudes des contractions rhythmiques des vaisseaux et du poumon ee chez les Holothuries. C. R. Soc. Biol., Paris, T. 55, pp. 4-1316. Johnson, R. A and Hall, R. W. 00. Variation and Regeneration and Synapta inherens. Science, N. S., 1900, p. 178. Lang, A ’96. Text-book of Comparative Anatomy. London, Vol. II. Ludwig, H 96. Echinodermen. Bronn’s Klassen u. Ord. des Tierreichs. Bd. II, Abt. 3, Buch 1 Morgan, T. H. ’01. Regeneration. The Macmillan Co., 1901. A. D 81. Zoology for High Schools and Colleges, 1881. Pearse, A. S. 08. Observations on the Behavior of Thyone briareus (Leseur). Biol. Bull., Vol. XV, pp. 259-286. 09. Antotomy in Holothurians. Biol. Bull., Vol. XVIII, pp. 42-49. SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION TERMS RELATING TO GENERIC TYPES In the field of biological taxonomy an important reform is in progress. The change is from fhe method of concepts to the method of types, in order that names may be applied with greater precision and permanence. Under the method of types we no longer think of the technical name of a plant or an animal as attaching primarily to a concept embodied in a description or definition, but as relating to the first representative of the group that became known to science. In determining the application of a specific name we go back to the original specimen or type on which the description was based. The original description has become secondary to the original specimen. In like manner generic names are treated as relating primarily to groups of species, with the original species as the generic type.* Without waiting to appreciate the fundamental nature of the change from concepts to types, many systematic workers took it for granted that generic types were to be determined by elimina- tions in much the same way that generic concepts had been treated, by gradual subdivision, restriction and removal of com- ponent groups. The general results of elimination were the same as under the method of concepts. The applications of many of the older generic names did not become definitely fixed, but remained dependent upon varying individual opinions of the validity of the work of later authors. It often happened that after elimination was accomplished only the doubtful or unidentifiable species remained to serve as generic types. Grad- ually it became apparent that the practise of elimination was inconsistent with the method of types, and could not insure stability in the application of names. Recourse was then had, especially by zoologists, to the arbitrary designation of generic 1 Cook, O. F., 1898, ‘‘ The Method of Types,’’ Science, N. S., 8: 513; also 1900, ‘‘The Method of Types in Botanical serene ama Science, N. S., 12: 475, and 1902, ‘‘Types and Synonyms,’’ Science, N. S., 15: 646. Swingle, Walter T., 1913, ‘*Types of Spon in E F Taxonomy,’’ Science, N, S., 37: 864. 308 No.569] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 309 types, the apparent object being to preserve the results of elimi- nation, even though the theory had to be abandoned. Probably it is only a question of time until the results of elimination will be discarded, as well as the theory, and replaced by the actual, historical types. A plan for determining the historical types of genera was adopted in 1907 in the American Code of Botanical Nomencla- ture, and other applications of the method of types are being rocognized by zoologists. Specialists in many groups are en- gaged in the study of generic types, and the need of a special terminology to facilitate work of this kind is becoming appar- ent. Thus in Bulletin 83 of the U. S. National Museum, ‘‘Type Species of the Genera of Ichneumon Flies,” by Henry L. Vie- reck, two new terms, ‘‘isogenotypic’’ and ‘‘monobasie,’’ are em- ployed in treating of the application of generic names to type species. The paper is of interest, not only to students of this group of insects, but also as an example of the tasks that con- front all taxonomists who appreciate the need of basing their work upon types. The distinctions to which the special terms refer are undoubtedly useful, and the possibilities of express- ing them in more convenient form are worthy of consideration. The word ‘‘isogenotypic,’’ is used with reference to cases where two or more generic names have been applied to the same type species. For this purpose a new term is not needed unless zoologists are unwilling to borrow from botanical nomenclature a more convenient method of treating the same class of cases. The botanical code provides a classification of synonyms, and applies the word ‘‘typonym’’ to a name that has to be rejected because an earlier valid name was proposed for the same type. The formation and use of typonym are in accord with a familiar analogy . As a preoccupied name becomes a homonym, it is easy to remember that the use of a preoccupied type results in a 2A different combination might have been expected, such as ‘‘autogeno- typie’? or ‘*deuterogenotypic,’’ since isogenotypic suggests the notion of equall¥ good. types or of equal numbers of types, instead of con- veying the idea of one and the same type, or of a second use of the same ype. Genera have been termed ‘‘isotypical’’? when they were described from more than one species, but all truly congeneric, on the assumption that such species would have equal standing as types. A still older use of the word ‘‘isotype’’ had reference to equal representation of a genus by similar or corresponding species in different geographical regions or geologic periods. See Schuchert, Charles, 1905, U. S. National Museum Bulletin 53, Pt. 1: 16. . 310 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. XLVIII typonym. A name based on a different type species, but con- generic with the type of an older genus, is termed a metonym. A name rejected for lack of an identified type is a hyponym, and one rejected for linguistic reasons, a caconym. All rejected names fall readily into these five classes. The other new term, ‘‘monobasic,’’ is used by Mr. Viereck to indicate genera with only one species at the original place of publication. In botanical literature the word ‘‘monotypic’’ is often employed in this sense, though also applied to genera that consist of only one species. If previous use disqualifies mono- typic, the same objection lies against monobasic. In addition to an older chemical meaning, the same word was employed several years ago in a biological sense, to describe a condition of descent in simple lines.* Apart from being preoccupied, the word mono- basic has a misleading implication, since under the method of types each generic name is referred to a single type species. The idea of a genus being based on many types is discarde with the method of concepts. Appreciation of this incongruity may explain why no such term as ‘‘symbasic’’ or ‘‘polybasic’’ is used in contrast with monobasic, to indicate genera that were first proposed in connection with more than one species. Evidently there is need of a simple and consistent terminology for indicating relations between generic names and type species. The normal relation under the method of types is the designa- tion of the type species at the original place of publication of the genus. Genera provided with types by original designation may be described as orthotypic, or normal-typed. With ortho- typic genera there is no occasion to raise the question of how many species were included at the original place of publication. 8 Cook, O. F., and Swingle, W. T., 1905, ‘‘ Evolution of Cellular Struc- tures,’’ Bull. 81, Bureau of Plant telist stry, U. S. Department of Agricul- tur re, p. 20. Plants or animals with specialized habits of asexual reproduc- secon tionary defines monobasis as follows: ‘‘The derivation of a stock from a single parentage by inbreeding, or by propagation of buds or cuttings; opposed to symbasis.’’ ee us the danger of ambiguity in using monobasis for nomenclatorial purposes is greater than in using monotypic, though it must be admitted that ma use of the word monotypic in two senses may sometimes result in confusion. Genera that were monotypic in the strictly nomenclatorial sense of being established in connection with one species may not be monotypic in the more general taxonomic sense of including only one species, No. 569] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 311 Genera that are not orthotypic fall into the two classes al- ready considered, those with a single species at the original place of publication, and those with two or more species. It is now generally agreed that when only one species was mentioned this should be accepted as the type. Such genera may be called haplotypic, or single-typed. When two or more species were in- cluded in the original treatment of a genus, and no type was designated, we have the problem of subsequent determination of the type, resulting in what may be termed a logotypic genus, that is, a genus with a rationally selected type species. The object of selection is to determine the historical type of the genus. Names must have definite applications, and historical applications of generic names can be made definite by ascertain- ing the historical types. The recognition of a new generic group is usually based on one leading or dominant species, with the others added as associate members. In many cases the generic type is intimated by the original author in dividing the genus into subgenera or sections, in illus- trating one of the species or citing illustrations published in earlier works, in naming the genus with particular reference to one of the species, in recording economic uses, or in giving geo- graphical or other indications of greater familiarity with one of the species. If the application of these or other historical cri- teria leaves more than one species eligible for selection, the first of the eligible species should be taken as logotype. In this way it is possible to develop a consistent system of type selection that will commend itself as reasonable and give the same results in the hands of different students.* Simply taking the first species under a generic name as the type would viet establish more of the generic names in their historical places than the method of elimination, which accepts the last of the original species left in the genus as the type. Either of these methods of selecting types would e avoided by taking the historical considerations more directly into account, as in the American Code si peaa cal Nomenclature. Probably a more sat- isfactory system for associating generic names wit their historical types could be developed by ait study of the problem. A policy of refusing to revive generic names that were not directly associated with binomial spe- mitigated priority. In proposing lists of ‘‘nomina utique conserv vanda advance of any provision for the definite application of names, European botanists have demonstrated one more way to put the cart before the horse. 312 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVII In addition to the three ways of associating generic names with their type species, there are many cases where generic names have been applied to groups that do not include the type, or any of the original species. Formal assignments of errone- ous types also occur when generic names are not traced back to their original places of publication, or when ineligible species are designated as types. In dealing with the synonymy of genera previously treated under names that belong to other groups it will be convenient to have a distinctive term for this class of cases. Such misplaced names, applied to groups that do not contain the true type, may be indicated as pseudotypic, or false-typed.® It should be expected that more critical analysis of taxonomic problems would lead to more definite distinctions and more pre- cise terms. The older terminology was developed to facilitate the study of names, whereas it is now apparent that provision must be made for the study of types as another formal branch of biological taxonomy. Nomenclature has a history of three hundred years while systematic typology is only beginning. To gain further insight into these typological problems is obviously more important than to attempt premature applications of par- tial solutions. It may take fifty or a hundred years to transfer Failure to regulate the application of names is the fundamental defect of the Paris and Vienna codes, and is hardly to be cured without thorough re- cas sae the palm genus Martinezia, as treated by Kunth, Martius, and many later writers as relating to Martinezia caryotefolia and its immediate relatives, was pseudotypic, for this species does not appear to be congeneric with any of the five species originally referred to Martinezia by Ruiz and Pavon. Hence it has been proposed to a ee this pseudotypie use of Mar- tinezia by a new generic name, Tilm (See Bull. Torrey Bot. Club, 28: 65.) The five original species of Maries belong to three natural groups, now recognized as distinct families, the first two species to the Cocacex, the third species to the Acristacee and the others to the e nosed The third species, M. ensiformis, should be taken as logotype of Martinezia be- cause the figures used to illustrate the generic characters evidently orii a member of the family Acristaceæ. Another reason for excluding the cocoid species from consideration as type is that the ey are mentioned as deviating from the ‘‘essential characters of the genus,’’ in connection with the original description. The rule of the Vienna code, to the effect that the name of a subdivided genus should go with the majority cf the species, would carry the name Martinezia over to the family Chamedoreacew. The making of such a rule shows that many European botanists were still working under the method of concepts, and were not copra to think of generic names as inseparably connected with type spec! No. 569] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 313 the whole structure of biological taxonomy to the new founda- tion of types. To suppose that any permanent advantage can be gained by elaborating defective methods under forms of legis- lative enactments or judicial decisions is to show a limited ap- preciation of the nature of the subject and of its historical de- velopment. As long as legislation and interpretation are based on inadequate study, they can represent, at most, only a tem- porary consensus of opinion, for it is of the very nature of science to condemn and throw aside any doctrine or method that has proven inadequate or fallacious. TERMS RELATING TO SYNONYMS The following classes of synonyms were recognized in 1907, in the American Code of Botanical Nomenclature :* Homonym.—A name rejected because of an earlier applica- tion of the same name to another genus. Typonym.—A name rejected because an older name was based on the same type. Metonym.—A name rejected because an older valid name was based on another species of the same genus. Hyponym.—A name not associated with a type." 6 Bulletin of the Torrey Botanical Club, 34: 167, 1907. 7 Much confusion would be avoided by a consistent policy of withhold- ing recognition of generie names that have not been associated with type species. Thus the name Acoeloraphe, proposed by Wendland in 1879 in an analytical key to genera of fan-palms (Bot. Zeitung, 37: 147), was not as- sociated with a type, though evidently relating to a species mentioned in the Same paper as ‘‘ Brahea serrulata.’’ This Florida palm differs from the Mexican type of Brahea in the leaf characters assigned to Acoeloraphe in rop by Hooker f. in 1883 for ‘‘ Sabal sormin Rh. o ee (Genera Plantarum, 3: 926). All subsequent writers have accepted Hooker’s name, and Acoel- oraphe should remain under Serenoa as a hyponym. Nothing has tended so strongly to bring the principle of priority into disrepute as the incontinent revival of abortive names, to replace properly agrees names in current use. No species was referred to Acoeloraphe until 1907, when Beccari (Webbia, 2: 107 ) applied the name Acoeloraphe Gigni to a Cuban mem- ber of a genus that had been described in 1902 under the name Paurotis, a Bahaman species, Pawrotis androsana, being the type (Mem. Torrey Bot. Club, 12: 21). This transfer of the name Acoeloraphae to the genus Paurotis was followed by Sargent in 1911 (Trees and Shrubs, 2: 117), but Beceari’s genus Acoeloraphe is a metonym of Paurotis, and is also pseudo- 314 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII TERMS RELATING TO TYPE SPECIES Orthotype.—Type by original designation. A species desig- nated as type in connection with an original publication of a generic name. A genus whose type was-formally designated at the original place of publication is orthotypic. Haplotype.—Type by single reference. A single species re- ferred to a genus at the original place of publication, and on this account accepted as the type. A genus proposed with refer- ence to a single species is haplotypic. Logotype.—Type by subsequent determination. The histori- cal type of a genus, selected from two or more original species. A genus whose type is selected from two or more original species is logotypie. Pseudotype.—Erroneous indication of type. A species erro- neously indicated as the type of a genus. A genus treated on the basis of an erroneous type, or so as to exclude the true type, is pseudotypie. O. F. Cook BUREAU OF PLANT INDUSTRY, U. S. DEPARTMENT OF AGRICULTURE, March 13, 1914 typic, because of the original application of the name to Serenoa. Two species of Paurotis are supposed to exist in Florida, one that is identi- fied with the Cuban P. wrightii (Grisebach & Wendland) and a local species, P. arborescens (Sargent). NOTES AND LITERATURE LINKAGE IN THE SILKWORM MOTH ONE of the most striking recent developments in the study of genetics is the discovery of linkage in many of those forms which were supposedly thoroughly worked out. The most recent ex- ample is a very interesting paper by Y. Tanaka,’ entitled “‘Gametic Coupling and Repulsion in Silkworms.’’ The data presented in this paper demonstrate the existence in the silk- worm moth of a group of four pairs of linked genes. Following Tanaka’s nomenclature we may designate these genes as follows: N, which differentiates the larval color pattern known as ‘‘normal’’ from that called ‘‘plain’’; S, occurring in larve having the ‘‘striped’’ pattern, and epistatie to N; M, the differ- entiator for the ‘‘moricaud’’ larval pattern, also epistatic to Y, the gene which differentiates caterpillars with yellow blood and yellow cocoons from the recessive whites. Of the six possible combinations of these genes, taken two at a time, all but NM and SM were made, and all showed linkage. F, ‘“‘eoupling’’ tests, i. e., from matings where both dominants entered the cross from ‘the same P, individual, were made for SY and for MY. In each case there occurred cross-overs, or new combinations of the characters, in such proportions as to lead Tanaka to suppose the ratio of parental combinations to cross- overs among the gametes to be about as 7:1. ‘‘Repulsion”’ (where one dominant entered from each P, individual) results were obtained for NS and for NY. In neither case did any double recessives (cross-overs) appear, though over 3,000 caterpillars were obtained in the case of NY, and 224 in the case of NS. From these data Tanaka concludes that the repulsion was complete in these two cases. It has, however, been pointed out by Morgan? that such results will be obtained if the linkage is com- plete in one sex only. In Drosophila such ‘‘repulsion’’ crosses never produce double recessives in F,, and it has been shown that this is due to complete linkage in the male, crossing over being frequent in the female between some pairs of genes. In order to test this possibility it is necessary to mate doubly hetero- zygous individuals to double recessives, when the gametic ratio is obtained directly and without the complications present in most F, results. It so happens that Tanaka reports two such crosses, one for each sex, though he does not recognize their im- 1 Jour. Coll. Agr., Tohoku Imper. Univ., Sapporo, Japan, V, 1913, 2 Science, N. S., XXXVI, 1912. 315 316 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vowu. XLVII portance in this connection. When a male heterozygous for S and for Y, one dominant having been derived from each parent (SysY), was mated to a doubly recessive (sysy) female, there were produced 63 Sy and 65 sY—no cross-overs. A female heterozygous also for S and for Y, but having them ‘‘coupled’’ (SYsy), was mated to a male sysy, and produced 215 SY and 188 sy—again no cross-overs. Yet that crossing over may occur between these two pairs of genes is shown by the fact that the ‘‘coupling’’ F, results indicated a gametic ratio of about :1:1:7. We are, therefore, still left in the dark as to whether crossing over occurs in only one sex, or in both. But it is certain that the strength of linkage in this case is not always the same— a point of great interest and importance. Similar cases have been reported by Baur? in Antirrhinum, by Punnett* in the sweet pea, and by the writer® in Drosophila. Tanaka refers to his case as differing from previously reported eases of linkage in animals in that the sex differentiator is not one of the genes involved, and in that the linkage is sometimes only partial. However, he refers several times to a paper by Morgan® in which it is clearly shown that three of the sex-linked genes in Drosophila also show partial linkage to each other, inde- pendently of their sex-linkage. Punnett,’ in referring to the same paper, has said, ‘‘Morgan’s experiments with Drosophila suggest coupling of some kind between factors for eye color and shape of wing, though both of these factors may show sex-limited inheritance in other families.’’ A study of the data referred to, or of any of the similar data on Drosophila since published, will show that these genes always show sex-linkage, and that at the same time they always show linkage to each other when both can be followed in the analysis. The two phenomena are not mutu- ally exclusive, but both are always present. Both Tanaka (in a footnote) and Punnett refer to the latter’s ease in rabbits as the first example of linkage in animals not involving sex. If the linkage between sex-linked genes is, for some strange reason, not considered to belong in this category, there are still at least two cases which antedate Punnett’s slightly. A few months before Punnett’s paper appeared I had suggested? the possibility of linkage in mice. It now seems rather probable that the relation in both mice and rabbits may really be that of 3 Zeits, f. ind. Abst.-u. Vererb.-Lehre., VI, 1912. 4 Jour. Genet., III, 1913. 5 Science, N. S., XX XVII, 1913. 6 Jour. Exp. Zool., XI, 1911. 7 Jour. Genet., II, 1912 (Nov.). $ AMER. NAT., XLVI, 1912 (June). No. 569] NOTES AND LITERATURE 317 triple allelomorphism. For this reason I am inclined to assign priority to Morgan and Lynch,’ whose paper on linkage of genes in Drosophila which are not sex-linked appeared after my own paper and before Punnett’s. CoLUMBIA UNIVERSITY A. H. STURTEVANT NABOURS’S BREEDING EXPERIMENTS WITH GRASSHOPPERS IN a recent paper, Nabours (’14) describes breeding experi- ments that he has been carrying on for some years with grouse locusts of the genus Paratettiz. His work is of special interest in showing that in a wild species there exists a number of distinct types that show alternative inheritance of a particular kind. His paper may be summarized as follows: 1. Nine distinct, true breeding forms of Paratettix were col- lected ‘‘in nature.’’ These ‘‘species’’ (as Nabours is inclined to consider them) ‘‘are mainly distinguished by their striking color patterns. ”? 2. When an individual of one of these species is mated to one of a different species the hybrid character of the offspring is apparent at once, in that ‘‘all the characters of each parent are represented in the F, hybrid.’ In other words, the hybrid is in a certain sense an intermediate, and ‘‘the terms dominant and recessive’’ are probably not ‘‘applicable at all. This point, while of little theoretic importance, has a practical value in that the zygotic constitution of any hybrid can be recognized without further breeding tests. 3. With one exception, each color pattern factor was found to behave as an allelomorph to any other color pattern factor. _ 4. The various lengths of the wings and pronotum are appar- ently not inherited, as such but are determined by environmental factors, especially such as tend to prolong or to shorten the length of larval life. It appears that Nabours confuses the relation of the facts men- tioned under 3, and that he supposes this to be the ordinary behavior of ‘‘mendelizing characters,” for he says: The essential result of these experiments has been the extension of this principle [Mendelian inheritance] to a considerable number of types of a phylogenetically low group of ametabolous insects. To be sure, he recognizes that other workers in genetics have ‘an attitude quite different from his, and he takes some little pains to make clear his own point of view. To quote again (p. 142): ® Biol. Bull., XXIII, 1912 (Aug.). 318 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII The existence of unit characters in the De Vriesian sense does not appear to have been as clearly demonstrated as that of alternative in- heritance . . . and the interpretations are at great variance. Thus, one group of authors [reference made to Bateson, Doncaster, and Tower] recognize characters in organisms that ean be replaced by other char- acters when the proper crosses are made, . . . while on the other side there are those [references to Whitman and Montgomery] who believe that the organism as a whole is the only unit and that there are no actual unit characters. Again he says (p. 169) : No character of one parent species is ever replaced in the F, hybrid by any character of the other parent. All the characters of each parent are represented in the F, hybrid. It follows then that these grass- hoppers do not exhibit characters which by crossing can be replaced by other different characters; the whole pattern appears to be the only unit. There is no real conflict between Whitman’s idea and the accounts given by students of Mendelism, for the latter realize that far-reaching somatic effects may result from a single factor, and the composite character of the hybrid is not an uncommon osourrenda. Nabours identite a particular pattern with the ‘‘organism as a whole,” but since his evidence relates here to color patterns only, nothing is gained by the introduction of such a vague phrase as the ‘‘organism as a whole.’’ Specifically he shows that the hereditary differences between any two types can be explained on the assumption of a single differential for each ase. With reference to the antithesis presented by Nabours, it must be recognized that the modern literature of Mendelian heredity affords innumerable instances where two or more characters entering from one parent and their allelomorphs from the other, reappear in the F, generation in new combinations. If we assume with Nabours that each of the eight color patterns are represented by a characteristic condition of the ‘‘germinal material,’? we may use his terms A, B, C, D, E, F, H or I to symbolize this ‘‘germinal material’? for the various color patterns. As Nabours uses the terms, an individual homozygous for A is represented simply by A, and a hybrid between A and B by AB. In ordinary usage, the homozygous form would be represented as AA and its germ cells by A. This is a minor matter. Ordinary usage has the advantage of being more consistent. According to Nabours, then, A mated to B gives AB; B mated to F gives BF; C mated to E gives CE, ete. In gametogenesis these factors segregate, so that, for example, BA gives germ cells No. 569] NOTES AND LITERATURE 319 A and B; BF gives B and F, ete. In other words, he treats the matter as if he were dealing with a system of multiple allelo- morphs, though he nowhere specifically calls them such. From this point of view there are eight distinct allelomorphs con- cerned with color pattern any two of which may constitute a pair; in any zygote two allelomorphs (perhaps alike, perhaps unlike) will be present, and in any gamete only one of the eight will normally occur. With one exception of which I will treat later, all of Nabours’s results can be explained by this hypothesis. This sort of explana- tion is not new. (Shull (’11), de Meijere (710), Sturtevant (713) and others have used it to explain results obtained in Lychnis, Papilio, rabbits and other forms, and it will almost un- doubtedly be shown to apply satisfactorily in still other cases. The exception just mentioned occurred in the cross which Nabours describes at the bottom of page 156 (e). Here a male of the constitution CE was mated to a female of the constitution BI. On Nabours’s theory, the gametes of the male should carry C or E, but not both, and the gametes of the female should carry B or I, but not both. The union of the two kinds of sperms with the two kinds of eggs should give four classes of offspring, and these were in fact obtained; viz., 12 BC, 11 BE,7 CI,10 EI. But there appeared also one individual BEI. Nabours ’s explanation of the case is that perhaps the BI ‘‘female parent gave at least one gamete containing the factors for the patterns of both her parents and that this double character gamete was fertilized by one of the E gametes which came from the CE male.’’* Let us see whether this is the most probable interpretation. As Sturtevant has pointed out, for any case to which the idea of multiple allelomorphism is applicable, an equally valid ex- planation may be found in ‘“‘complete linkage’’ of the factors concerned. To decide in any case between the two explanations would be impossible. If, however, linkage were not complete, a ‘‘eross-over’’ class or ‘‘recombination’’ class might occur, and this would suffice to rule out the explanation based on multiple allelomorphs. Such a ‘“‘eross-over’’ class perhaps is furnished by the BEI individual, The demonstration of this may be given by the use of symbols, as follows: Let us assume that A is the allelomorph of a, B that of b, C of c, D of d, F of f, I of i, ete., making eight pairs of allelomorphs altogether. Assume that each gamete of any individual carries 1 This explanation is essentially similar to that advanced by Bridges (713) to explain certain peculiar results in Drosophila. Bridges assumed that in gametogenesis the two X-chromosomes of a white-eyed female failed to segre- gate (in Bridge’s terminology, non-disjunction occurred), and passed over together into one gamete. 320 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII one allelomorph of each pair, and that the eight factors thus present in a gamete form a linked group, tending to segregate as a unit in gametogenesis. Thus Nabours’s form A would give gametes of the form Abcdefhi. AB would give gametes of only two forms, one corresponding to A and the other to B, viz., Abcdefhi and aBcdefhi. Two other forms are possible, formed by the exchange of A with B, and of a with b, but these will not occur if linkage is complete. In dealing with the hybrid AB in practise the factors cdefhi would not be put into the formule, as they are alike in all gametes. These rules would apply similarly to all other species and hybrids. Therefore in the case in which the BEI individual occurred, we would represent the male parent, which Nabours designated CE, by bCei—bcEi, and its gametes by bCei and bcEi. The female parent, which Nabours designates BI, we would represent by Bcei—bceI, and its gametes would be Bcei and bcel if linkage were complete. If linkage were not complete there would occasionally be formed gametes bcei and Bcel. One of these latter (Bcel) was probably formed and fertilized by a sperm of the type bcEi, thus giving rise to the BEI individual. No gametes corresponding to bcei appear to have been fertilized, though of course we do not yet know what the appearance would be of an individual so formed. This matter would be easy to test, and it is to be hoped the cross may be repeated. If then BEI forms should appear again and in these when mated to other forms the factors B and I should be found to stay together to the same extent as they before sepa- rated, it would show that close linkage, rather than multiple alle- lomorphism explains this particular instance. It may be, too, that both linkage and multiple allelomorphism play a part in the production of these phenomena. In any case it seems that the test is at hand, and not difficult to perform, excepting in so far as there are practical difficulties connected with the rearing of the grasshoppers in sufficient numbers to cover the point. LITERATURE CITED. Bridges, ©. B. 1913. Non-disjunction of the Sex Chromosomes of Droso- phila. Jour. Exp. Zool., Vol. 15. de Meijere, J. ©. H. 1910. Ueber Jacobsons Ziichtungsversuche bezüglich Polymorphismus von Papilio Memnon L. 9, ete. Zts. ind. Abst.- Vererb.-Lehre, Vol. 3 Nabours, R. K. 1914. Studies of Inheritance and Evolution in Orthoptera. I. Jour. Genet., Vol. 3 Shull, G. H. 1911. heversihte Sex Mutants in Lychnis dioica. Bot. Gaz., Vol. LII. Sturtevant, A. H. 1913. The Himalayan Rabbit Case with some Consid- erations on Multiple Allelomorphs. Am. Nat., Vol. XLVII. COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY Joun S. DEXTER VOL. XLVIII, NO. 570 JUNE, 1914 od rf H bet HI. ad : ares and Evolution. By X - THE AMERICAN NATURALIST A MONTHLY JOURNAL Devoted to the Advancement of the Biological Sciences with Special Reference to the Factors of Evolution CONTENTS Page pies nen by Hybridization and Mutation. “Professor — JOHN H. ii Rereaiey of Bristles in the Common gar ges ie Phen Study í of Factors verning Distribution. PHINEAS W. 339 Pagsilogent eS ana CASIN peace in Alfalfa Breeding. GEO. 1 g=3, 3, albe, 3. 1 sae a ; 3. 1913-D, L. cæsar 9 = 3, 2, 2, 3 (the rapiodgad normal for pa species), taken at Bussey Institution, May 5, 1913 gav 55 dg = 3, 2, 2, 3. usai 2, 2, 3 4 gg = 3, 1, 2, 3. 1 Q= ac, 2, 2, 3. 1 g==3, 2,b 2433 =3, 1,13 1 d=3, 2 1 The flies of this mating are not averaged with the others, as it is possible that this species may be different in its variability from L. sericata. It is noteworthy, however, that here also reduction favors the male more than the female. ' No. 570] FACTORS GOVERNING DISTRIBUTION 343 general reduction tends considerably to favor the males, while addition favors the females to a slight extent. In my previous paper (p. 264) is given in detail a record of the progeny of a female L. sericata (1912-c) lacking both of the first and the right second post-acros- tichal (3, 2, 1, 3). These were inbred to the third gen- eration, in all cases brother being mated with sister in an attempt to analyze the stock as thoroughly as possible and to reduce heterozygosis of factors. Here again, due 4 Mr. Harold D. Fish has kindly furnished me the following note: ‘‘The importance of mating sisters with brothers for a long series of generations in the experiments aimed to detect Mendelizing units of inherit- ance and analyze groups of them, quite generally seems to have been over- looked. As first shown by Castle (’03), random mating of the individuals of successive generations beyond F, tends to produce in each generation a population with the sam e per ag of E and heterozygosis as is gous for one faktor of a beak tats pair, 25 per cent. homozygous for the other factor, and 50 per cent. heterozygous for both. Such a system of random matings often has been confused with the more restricted. system of motini sisters with brothers. AE ip peny that if A and B are an allelomorphic pair the F, zygotes, resulting from a mating of AA with BB, will be Further, if pas are all females and are mated in all possible ways with the same number and kinds of males, one sixteenth of the matings will be 4A with AA, and one sixteenth will be BB with BB. One eighth of the TAA then, will be homozygous and produce only homozygous young, which, be- cause of the restricted system of mating only sisters with brothers, ay pro- duce, in turn, only homozygous matings. The remaining m This would mean that the reoht ai of heterozygous matings between in äi- viduals of the F, ge eneration would be (7/8)"*. “Accordingly one would ex- pect an automatic increase in y a The expectation is justified al- Though the figures are misleadin De. Raymond Pearl first published the figures exactly expressing the 344 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII to high mortality, selection as might have been desired has been impossible. A detailed account of this strain is given in Table II. In recording any mating of this strain the letter c denot- ing the entire strain, is followed by F,, F., ete., denoting the generation from which the mated flies hive been chosen. This symbol is then followed by a, b, or c, denot- ing the first, second, or third mating, respectively, of the generation indicated. Thus mating cF,b is the second mating of the second inbred generation of strain 1912—c. This method of recording matings has been followed throughout my work. Several points of interest are to be noted in this strain but it is thought best to present the remaining data on reduced strains before proceeding to a discussion of this matter. Strict inbreeding has been followed in the strain re-. corded below. In no case have there been either cousin- matings or outcrossings. 1913-4, L. sericata 9 =3, ac, ac, 3, taken at Bussey Institution, Forest Hills, Mass., May 6, 1913, gave F, 96 JJ = 3, 3, 3, 3. Bers we 3, 3, 3. 1 gg =3, ac, ac, 3 1 Yo 3, ac, 3, 3. 2 dg = 3, ae, 3, 3. S99==5, 2,3; pi 2 dd = 3, 3, ac, 3 1 an 2, ac, 3 4 $$ =3, 2, 3, 3. pe am, 8. 3,8, as the per cent. of the allelomorphice factors which are homozygous in the average individual of that generation. Because Dr. Pearl in his October met referred rae to a paper by Dr. E. M. East ( ie, on ‘‘ Hetero- zygosis in Evolution and Plant Breeding.’’ I gave Dr. East my figure ex- Bele pa per pated of homozygosis in successive coma resulting from matings of sisters with brothers. Dr. Pearl’s correction followed a matics of random matings in each generation to a case where sisters always had been mated with brothers. The percentages, as computed, were pub- lished by Dr. Pearl for the following generations: P,—100 per cent., F,—0 per cent., F,—50 per cent., F,—50 per cent., F,—62.5 per cent., F,—68.25 per cent., F,—75 per cent., F;—79.687 per cent., F,—83.594 per cent, Fy— 86.719 per cent., F~—89,258 per cent. Previous to giving these figures to Dr. East I computed the number of generations megri to reduce heterozy- gosis to less than one half of one per cent. and found this condition first realized in the F., generation, which is 99.553 or pe: — e The importance of these figures in work of this nature is quite obvious.’ FACTORS GOVERNING DISTRIBUTION 345 No. 570] ot |_ 81 Ot E 1 Oy AF 2] Sty O18 4: TE 1 OL | St 7 Bt) oe Fale Sea Tats -oL Lá SI 0 O01107 T0040 £61 t Il I 0 eg | CRN Ssh flags ama [BulIOU= Q79 Wor] ty (pg) &% OPO aie) Ost HSE 4 a 9 9 OLT £91 Stars eet ge So[BUley ewou g (PZ) S'e : X eyeur puou 8 G. IOT F 0 = DJI UIO} ty 0 T De t Prr Lodra zet | o I 6 Mo eer a et aed ewou = 91,79 WOI t 0 j 0 \PaZslo;/oleloloilr | 28 0 Z g Z CG. We “+ aud [wuLLOU= qiyo woa ty OO 0 0;0;/0/0/0;oj0/] at | o 0 0 0 L oo on aed Buou = D II WOY t 0 0 0 001010000 | g 0 0 0 0 a 5 Gee S Ss EIZ ‘g) = 19y}owu wory y é 2 é 2 ô| ejó] jól] é 2 é © fe] © pe pe] at {uo Ta SOTTT S[@1jU90-osaoqy |— Soe OCS “501g |, pesvesouy pasear q [vULioN aia << POSBOIOUT,, UL s[ByoIjso10y IL NEW Aenea pas i oa A E Sa ta S dee a: ; JO 19vIvyp pur suopesauan sera « PeseaI00q » UL Jurgowa səapsug səpsug She ag SƏFA jo po adan UUIN[OD ISI UL paps0deyy vy sSurEyy moiy AueSo01g GIGI ‘8 ISAÐNV ‘Nossog ‘moog apVaUVvy THL LY NAAVL ‘(g ‘T ‘g “¢) & 01021438 “7 IZIGI NIVULS A0 axoomy Il Widvi 346 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII 2 from AF,a= ioc ac, 3, 3. == 5, 2, 8, 8: g 1, 3 Sg = 3, ac, 2, 3 5 dd ama, 2, ac, 3 Log 8, 2,2, 3 5 dg =3, 2, 3, 3 238 8, 2, 3 ol BRED RED HH OD Ww +0 +C+0+0 +0+0+0+0+0+0 Gs ea oa GS OD +0 +0+0+0+0+0+0 Hee MS Sl! RS ORR MR SOE a a R e .- ~ Sa `~ > oO Pair segregated July 12; larve July 25 99 ae S 6 a5 == 3, 3; 8, 3 7 2=3, 2, ae, 3 42 == 8; ót; BC, 10 4223, 2, 2,3. 9 18 == 3, ne, 8,5 7 728, 8,8, 3 16 1 3, 3, ac, 3 5 19, 8,2, 8 5 2 =ne ae, 2,9 1 O== 3; ac, 873 from AF,.b = d = 3, ae, 2, 3 j Q =3, abe, 2, 3. Pair segregated July 12; larvae July 25. 3c. n dd 16 45 = 3, 3, 3, 4 1==8, 3, 2, 34 9 = 3, ac, ac, 3 0 1==38, abe, ac, 3 8 0==3, ac, 5, 3. 0 $==3, 3, abe, 3 7 Li 88): dey 8. 0 3=3, abe, abe, 3 8 == Ot, oy Os 1 0==3, b, 3, 3. 9 i= 3, 2) At, 3. 1 0=3, ać, abe, 3. ‘ 3 OSS. Fy p B. 0 1==8, abe, 3, 3. 2 1==8, 2, 3, 3. = g=3 3, from AB. P 14; Ag == 8, a6, 8, rom AF,a. ps) ee August 14; from APa = 19 = 3, ac, ac, 3, larve August do D 33 e 6 32 = 3, 3, 3, 3 4 T= 9, 2, 3; 3. 24 11 ==8, ac, ae, 3 1 p= B, 3, 2, 3. 4 20== 3, ae, 3, 3. 1 0=8, ac, 1, 3. 6 $==38, 8, ac, 3. 0 T= 8, ae, 3, 9. 9 "6-29, ae, 2, 3: 1 0= 3, ac, bee, 3 6 3, 2, ac, 3. 0 1228, 2, 2, 8, 3 =n. 2, 2, Sy 0 1= 3, 3, abb from AF,b = and ie 2, ac, 3, from AF,a. Pair segregated August 13; larve August 2 dd Go n 0 18 =3, 3, 3, 3 13 2==5,'2,' ae, 3 10 == 3, ac, ac, 3 2==5, 3, 3.3. 4 5=23, ac, 8, 3 1 72B, 2,8, 3. 2 == 3, 3, ae, 1 4==3, 8, 3,3 3 from AF = g — Eea 2, 2, 3, from AF,a. Pair segregated August 13; larvæ August No. 570] FACTORS GOVERNING DISTRIBUTION 347 Go. oy do fs 6 15 = 3, 3, 3, 3. 2 15 = 3, 2, 3, 3 10 7 = 3, ac, ac, 3 3 8 = 3, 3, 2, 3 1 7 =3, ae, 3, 3 0 1 = ac; h 2, ac 10 12==3, 3, ae, 1 0=83, 2, ace, 8 3 == 3, ac, 2, 2 1==3, ace, ac, 3 t fr as a 1 0= 3, ace, 3, 3 5 4= from AF d= Jg aka yes c, ac, 3, from AF,a, Pair segregated August 13; nels died August 18 mes another with same chetotaxy put in; larve Septem ber 22 od. T7 0 == 3, 3, 3, 3. 1 be= S, ac; 2, 3. 1 2==3, ae, 3, 3. 1 0 = 3, 3, aabe, 3. 3 The tá it 1018 2. eooriei in tabular form is given in Table III. We are now in a position to consider the nature of re- duction of bristles in Lucilia sericata. It is evident from Table I (record of first generation flies), that reduction and addition of bristles are both hereditary. It is further evident from Table ITI, (inbred strain), that reduction yields readily to selection. This effect may be expressed by making the number of bristles’ lost the numerator of a fraction of which the denominator is the number of bristles normal. We then have a ratio for each generation of 1913—A as follows: OES fı. 3892 7 0.006 = .010, F; - fen = 0.093 + .003, 99 532.5 Fy - 1788 ~ 0.055 = .004, Fi: 5100 > ure + .003. It may be readily seen by glancing at these figures that selection has a very rapid effect. It also appears that as we pass from F, to F, the effect of selection gradually diminishes. This may be expressed by dividing the above decimals for each generation by that of the preceding generation. ba thant Pe a a o «= 1,69, K oom >e aOR Fy _ 0.104 _ es 0.093 11k, The reason for this decrease in the effect of selection in the later generations is that as the selection advances the majority of the flies become reduced in two bristles only. [ Vou. XLVIII THE AMERICAN NATURALIST 348 € L | € |0|0)@|9892| Zh \seT 16r srj € | 9 T | T | 008| 11% hoa Oe i ae tt SOL = | ea 0 o tok do folates 9 te Tae gee olo lolin eee ee ogy woa | T i ‘g ‘on ‘oe ‘g= & pue (0 | | = pty wor tI 0 I y OE toloto 9 Z9 IEF 18.| OFT if $ 0 | 0 Pao IST 9 o May MoI | ‘e ‘S's ‘€= 6 pue p =98 V Worf rT sees e es eope ay WOJ 0 0 0 0 41 0-} 04.0 : St 19 (0% OF | 6 Dto 0 | O |83 | 6% ST 0 | ‘98 ‘S‘g= § pue p | | ER =qay mol rg Z 0 T |Q@8T) E E 969 94 J98 |88 | EZT Ea 0 | 0 Ja 89 gujo "oyy utoj | | ‘g ‘ow ‘ov ‘g= b | | 5 tg ‘oR ‘om 2 | | =p y Wor + ZI 0 0 0 |0/0|T {Sr | STOTT |e [08 | SZI 0 0 0 | 0 0P 22 SF 9T] L g ‘9qQ0 ‘g= 8 tg tz ‘on ‘g= yp = y y Woy My 0 0 0 0 LO1OPTE?: of EZI 19S [PE | STZ 0 0 0 | 0 (04 OL Se 9 |g ‘ov ‘ou‘g= 6 pue, = OY Wor tT 8 Z I 0 |0/0|0/ST-} 9°8S [2 0r RO I I T | O |9T | 9 [Eo g et eee : tø 'o tog tpa ve) =p y Woy FT 0 0 0 6°10; 0763.7 L e 18.| 1% 0 0 0 | pe 18i Brog ette (g ‘98 ‘ow “g) | = Joy},OUL wory Ty ò | 2 6 olej] ej 6 2 48/2" é 2 61/216 | P1 bl eS e z R syo S[eI}ua9 Aue elba sora -0si0(] peatnacane “Box d renpeesent pesveriouy | pesveroeq | [VION iene PZ TIBMUS |. PesBetouy ,, UF 1870. Ul pasvetouy 194081840 PUB suopean POPPY Sos ug puer pasear q sənsug SAHA c PISBIALQ ,, UF BUPATI SONSHA ButMoysg sərf JO 1əquny umn[oD ISAT UL pəpiooay supe wos Luafosg SIGI ‘9 AVIV “SSY ‘STITT LSau0,7 ‘NOILALIGSNT AASSAJ AHL LV Nauvy, (E ‘ov ‘ov ‘g) $ 01048 `T ‘P-EIGT NIVULG 40 auoomy II wiav No. 570] FACTORS GOVERNING DISTRIBUTION 349 Rarely does a fly occur lacking more than two. In the few cases in which three or more bristles are lacking, the absence of the third acrostichals or of the dorso-centrals is as frequent as the absence of first and second acros- tichals. Why this should be is difficult to understand, as it would be expected that both first and both second post- acrostichals might frequently be lacking in the same fly, especially as flies asymmetrical for the loss of these bristles are common. A further point of interest lies in the fact that not only is number of bristles a hereditary matter, but their dis- tribution is also hereditary.. Thus from Table I (first- generation flies) we see that in general the first post- acrostichals tend to be reduced more than the second. This may be expressed as a fraction: First post-acrostichals lacking n 40.5 _ 1.19. Second post-acrostichals lacking 34 It is possible that this tendency to reduce the first post- acrostichal more than the second is evidence of relation- ship to L. cesar Linn., in which the absence of the former and the presence of the latter is the normal condition. Strain 1913—4 (Table III), however, gives Second post-acrostichals lacking 750 Considering the reduction in the first post-acrostichals Separately, we may express the effect of selection — as follows: First post-acrostichals lacking a 329 _ 0.43. Offspring. Parents. lst post-acros. First post-acros. lacking. Matings. lacking. OSEA -across. normal (2 per fly). A 0(?) a = 0.021 + .004. 26 AF,a 1 io 0.087 + .011. 60 AF.a 0 As == 0.111 £ .002. 33 — 0.093 + .010 AF,b 2 356 i 350 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII 64 AF aap Ewin + sa 0 34g 0-185 + .014. AF,b 2 op —=— 0.316 + .023. s 190 AF, 4 a0. .253 + .017. From these figures it is readily seen that reduction in the first post-acrostichals is not entirely consistent with the direction of selection. Let us test the same matter for reduction in the second post-acrostichals. Offspring. Parents Ist post-acros, First post-acros. lacking. Matings lacking First post-acros. normal (2 per fly) —8_ — ES A 2(?) 182 0.017 + .004. 63 Soha as + A¥,a 1 398 0.211 + .016. 193 AF,a 4 26 == 0.453 + 016. 147 AF .b 1.5 “a6 0.413 + .018, AF a 4 TOD = 0.420 + .179. 69- AF,b 2 “90° = 0.363 + .023 108 AF;¢ 0 “292 = 0.370 + .019. + In this ease also the results are not consistent with the direction of selection, although there is better agreement here than in the case of the first post-acrostichals. This is probably due to the fact that the numbers are larger. As regards the irregularities that do occur, I consider them as evidence of recombinations of multiple factors, insofar as they are not due to probable error. 1912—c (Table IT) is a strain that especially tends to lack the first post-acrostichals. Thus for the entire strain First post-acrostichals lacking K _ Second oe lacking 11` sll No. 570] FACTORS GOVERNING DISTRIBUTION 351 In the 137 offspring of a single mating of this strain, cF,c, there are 23 first post-acrostichals lacking, showing that it is due to this mating especially that the strain is so lacking in first post-acrostichals. It can not as yet be said that the factors governing the first post-acrostichals are altogether independent of those governing the second. That a certain degree of inde- pendence obtains is evident from a comparison of the ratio of reduction in first to reduction in second post- acrostichals in flies in general (Table I), with the same ratio for strain 1913—A. In the former case we have 40.5/34 or 1.19. In the latter we have 329/750, or 0.43. In order to establish the independence of the factors un- derlying these two tendencies it will be necessary to obtain, either by selection from a strain showing both tendencies or by breeding from wild stock, two strains, one tending to lack the first while retaining the second, and the other tending to lack the second while retaining the first. A point of interest in strain 1913—A is the presence of twelve small second post-acrostichals in the progeny of AF.,b in which the female had one of these reduced to half size. The progeny of AF,a in which there was total ab-, sence of these bristles showed either presence or absence of the same but no reduced bristles. In F, however, we have eight reduced bristles. The occurence of these small bristles in the progeny of certain matings is taken _ as an indication of recombinations of multiple factors, but the numbers are too small to establish this with cer- tainty. A glance at the tables shows that third post-acrosti- chals are rarely lacking. These are normally present in all related species, while in a few,—Cynomyia mor- tuorum, Musca domestica, Pseudopyrellia cornicina, and others, there is normally but one post-acrostichal, and this is always the last. Posterior dorso-centrals are very rarely absent. Thus in the 2,273 flies recorded in Table I only one had a single post-dorso-central missing. Reduction in post-acrosti- 352 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII chals among these is 79.5. Among the 1,206 flies of strain 1913—A there -are but three post-dorso-centrals gone. This latter is a highly reduced strain as regards post- acrostichals, lacking 1,081. This great reduction in acrostichals seems not appreciably to have affected the dorso-centrals, a fact which argues for the independence of the factors controlling the distribution of these two sets of bristles. Thus for flies recorded in Table I we have Post-acrostichals lacking _ 79.5 — 0.03 Number of Flies oe One post-dorso-central lacking. For flies in strain 19134 (Table III) we have Post-acrostichals lacking TB! 0.89 Number of Flies HATO .; Three post-dorso-centrals lacking. Among the 3,238 flies recorded in Tables I and III only four post-dorso-centrals are lacking, while among the 810 flies of strain 1912—¢ (Table IT) there are 13.5 lacking. The lack of post-acrostichals in this latter strain is 37. There are 9.5 dorso-centrals lacking in the progeny of the ‘trio, cF,a, among which there are only seven post-acros- tichals lacking. Thus we see that lack of post-dorso-centrals is in no way correlated with lack of post-acrostichals, but is evi- dently governed by distinct factors, VaRIATION By ADDITION oF BRISTLES A strain of Lucilia sericata, 1913—E, showed some interesting variations chiefly in the direction of addition of bristles. The mother was normal (3, 3, 3, 3) , taken at the Bussey Institution, March 19, 1913. The detailed ac- count of the strain follows: dd Y3 : 38 43=3, 3, 3, 3. 1 0=3, 3, able, 3. F, 2 fim BV oes eS No. 570] FACTORS GOVERNING DISTRIBUTION 353 3. 1 ? 2 P 3. 0 = labe, 3, 3, !abe. F, from EF.a= ¢ and 9? =3, 3, 3, 3. QF dd 318 251=3, 3, 3, 4 5= 3, albe, 3, 13 51=!abe, 3, 3, labe 0 = lalbe, 3, 3, labe 1 4 = !abe, 3, 3, 3 sl = 3, abc!, abe! 6 =3, 3, 3, labe 1 0= labe, albe, albe, la 1 =3 (a), 3, 3, 3 0 1= labe, atbe,, 3, labe 5 2=3, 3, 8 (a), 8 1 0==3, able, 3, 3 3 5 = 3, 3, albe, 3 1 1=albe, 3, 3, albe. $ 0 = 3, albe, aes 3. 0 = labe, 3, 3, !albe. 1 0=3, 3,3; 0 13, albe, 3, la 1 0=3, 8, gei 0 1 = labe, 3, 3 (a), la 1 0 = labe, albe, 3, 3 0 1= !abe, a!be, 3, la!be 1 0=3, 3, 3 (b), 3. 0 l=albe, 3, albe, albe 1 1==3, albe, albe, 3. 1 Ibe, 3, 3, la! 1 = lalbe, !a!be, falbe, lalbe. 1 Onn 3, $, 2, 0 is oo one. 1 == 3, ac, 3, 3 0 2=lalbe, 3, 3, la 1 0=8, ac, ac, 3. ‘ 0 1= tabe, b, albe, tae. 0 1-53, abe, 3, 3. 4 — f = 3, 3, 3, 3. from EFa= {8T} 3 albe, 3. Pair segregated, July 22; larvæ July aT 191 15) a 5, 1 = oe 3, 2 labe. 25 43= labe, 3, 3, labe 2 0 = 3, 13 4 = labe, 3, 3, 3. 1 (= tabe (a), 3, 3. 0 - J=88, 8, 8, labe 1 0O=8 (a), 3, 3 (a), 3. 0 1= labe, albe, 3, labe. 1 1= labe, 3, 3 (a), !abe. 1 2 = labe, 3, albe, !abe. 0 lbe, 3, 0 2 = labe, albe, albe, labe. from EF,b = and 9 = 3, 3, 3, 3. Pair segregated August 22. dg 41 57=3, 3, 3, 3. 2 0=3, albe, 3, 3. 0 =3 albe, 3, labe, 0 iz laby 3, 3, tabe. 1 028, abel, 8, 3. 0 1= labe, albe, albe, labe. A summary of this strain is given in Table IV. The points of interest to be noted in this table are as follows: There are many supernumerary bristles in the flies of this strain. The number of bristles added in the progeny of any mating is very variable and has no consistent relation to the visible character of the parents. Addition of bristles tends very much to favor the females, reduction still affecting the males. Despite the high ratio of bristles added, there are THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII 354 163 SSI S'I Lá ESP‘ I I SPT S8 I £ LGG L FAR Maneater Rasta i So Po OE STSIOL 8 € 0 0 FOL 0 0 £ £ 0 0 2g E A EAN E pres ol A ‘ared ewou = qA seme’ Je IIT € £ 0 I8€ I 0 €¢ 9g 0 0 OOT T61 g ‘oq t ‘g ‘g= 6 } ‘eg ‘e ‘eg ‘g= = DIY WOT tI OLT cL ‘0 Lá 602 0 I T6 tPF $ £ LEA BE Poa ee ee Ied ewou = Dga WOI t Z I 0 0 L¥L 0 0 T I 0 0 94 69 ee EEFC AEUR, HERA ELS nE €‘e‘e‘e=6 ‘g ‘oj, qu ‘g ‘g= 9 : = DIJA WOY ey 0 i 0 0 8 0 0 0 $ 0 0 EF Be tee Se pa ot aaa or é © é Ps é Le é Pa é 2 ô Pad sua : 2 pasverouy : F i a e » Ul sh 5) es mond pus AIR hors CBS: patents ER s3urnew JO 194981840 pur uopuIouap POPPY INSHA | -A08 SIST səpsug Baraoyg sət q JO saquny UUIN[OD ISI UI pepsoosy ssaywyy WoIy Audso01g SI6L ‘GI HOUYI “SSVI ‘STINET ISTO,T ‘NOILALIGSNI AUSSOG THL LV Navy, (¢ ‘ge ‘g ‘g) & 04091423S “7 ‘NIVALE M-QIGI AO TUOTA AI WTa&vVib No. 570] FACTORS GOVERNING DISTRIBUTION 355 nevertheless a few flies in the strain in which bristles are lacking. Bristles normally present may be lacking in individuals having additional bristles. GENERAL SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS Taking a general summation of all the bred material of Lucilia sericata, we find that reduction affects the males while addition affects the females. Of the 5,367 flies bred, 2,708 are males and 2,659 are females, giving practical equality. Reduction in the males is 748.5 bristles, while in the females it is only 455.5 bristles. As has been noted before the degree of reduction in the females is increased by the later generations of strain 1913—A, by reason of the fact that reduction rarely goes beyond the loss of two bristles in a single fly. Thus when most of the flies of a popula- tion become reduced to this extent it is evident that reduc- tion in the males would be but slightly in advance of that in the females. There are 210 bristles added in the males, while there are 343 added in the females. Thus addition affects the females more than the males. These figures for bristles added represent number of bristles, and thus no distinc- tion is made between bristles of large and bristles of small size. I wish to express my appreciation for the advice and criticism offered me in this work by Professor W. M. Wheeler, Messrs. H. D. Fish, S. G. Wright, and C. C. Little. | PHYSIOLOGICAL CORRELATIONS AND CLI- MATIC REACTIONS IN ALFALFA BREEDING?! GEO. F. FREEMAN ARIZONA AGRICULTURAL EXPERIMENT STATION Curmatic REACTIONS To the worker who attempts to apply the recognized laws of heredity to the actual operations of plant improve- ment many difficulties arise which heretofore have been largely avoided by students of pure genetics. Color and form characters are but little affected by the immediate ordinary environment and hence, for the sake of simplic- ity, are usually chosen by investigators of heredity. To the economic breeder, however, such characters are of but little consequence except in so far as they indicate phyletic relationships. Of greater importance to the breeder are those differences in yield and quality which are the re- sults of inherited, invisible, physiological powers within the plants, whereby each variety may respond differently in manner or degree to the same environmental stimulus. Those hereditary units which have to do with vegetative vigor, heat, cold and drought resistance, time of maturity, chemical structure, reproductive strength, etc., are as yet but little understood. This is largely due to the difficulty of exact experiments concerning them. This difficulty is occasioned by the complexity of the reactions of these hereditary forces with the external environment, and also by the direct influence of the development of one part of the plant upon that of some other part. The plant at ma- turity presents the resultant of its environmental reac- tions during development. The nature of these reactions 1 Read before the American Breeders’ Association, Columbia, S. C., Jan- uary 26, 1913. 356 No. 570] ALFALFA BREEDING 357 is determined by the structure of the vital forces within. These differences in vital structure may or may not be accompanied by visible morphological differences. Such cases of correlation have been known and used in selecting for qualities which they were thought to indicate. The much quoted example of the supposed correlation between the short-haired rachilla and high brewing quality in bar- ley is a case in point. It has been found, however, that, whereas, in one strain or race the correlation may hold, in another, the two characters are in no way related. Another case of similar nature is the coupling of cob and pericarp color in certain varieties of corn and their com- plete independence in others. Many other examples could be adduced to show that the coupling of two characters in a given race of plants is no indication that these same characters are inseparably linked in all races of the same species. These facts have greatly reduced the value for- merly ascribed to gametic correlations in plant breeding. Under our present knowledge, therefore, we must depend, for the most part, upon direct experimentation, rather than correlations, to discover the hereditary physiological characters of the varieties with which we are working. Any additional light, therefore, which may be had con- cerning the nature of such characters, together with meth- ods for the study of the behavior of the same in their rela- tion to each other and to their physical surroundings, will have not only a scientific value, but will also fill a distinct practical need. As an illustration of such a study we may now examine the data concerning the development, yield and chemical composition of forty-four regional varieties of alfalfa which were grown on the Experiment Station Farm at Phoenix, Arizona, during the season of 1910. In the case of this plant, which occupies the ground throughout the year and from which six or seven crops may be harvested during the growing period, the climatic factors include a long series of variations coincident with the changing sea- sons. Now, since every variety consists of its own pecul- 358 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII iar complex of hereditary physiological forces, each sensi- tive in its own manner and degree to the impinging ex- ternal stimuli, it is not surprising that the resultant (the gross climatic reaction) should be sharply different in the several varietal groups. The unequal effects upon the vegetative growth of the different varieties brought about by the climatic changes which occurred during the course of the summer may be exhibited by calculating the place variation in yield. This is best shown by correlating the first with each of the fol- lowing cuttings throughout the season. The result is a definite curve, beginning and ending high with a strong sag in the middle. | TABLE I PLACE VARIATION IN YIELD Cuttings 1 and 2 1 and 3 land 4 Correlation. + .75+ .04 + .68 + .05 + .33+ .09 + 36 oe 09 + 58 .07. These figures indicate the presence of some disturbing factor which reached its maximum intensity during the fourth and fifth cuttings, and to which certain plots were more sensitive than others. The average period through which the growth of these two crops extended was June 22 to August 27. The fact that these dates include the hottest portion of the summer strongly suggests temperature as the disturbing factor. The mean maximum temperature, mean minimum rela- tive humidity and the correlation between yield and water supplied are given in the following table: TABLE II TEMPERATURE, RELATIVE HUMIDITY AND WaTER SUPPLY Mean Max- Mean Mini Correlation Cutting) Dates pagan yo meas Periods imum T Tempera “ikelative pr rae a Supply 1 From March 23 to April 23 82.8 27.00 z From April 23 to May 23 sii +.10 3 | From May 23 to June 22 103.6 20.40 + .05 + .10 4 | From June 22 to July 23 104.8 : +-.40 + .09 5 | From July 23 to August 27 104.4 30.00 + .21+.10 6 From August 27 to October 5 102.0 95.18 ra + .10 No. 570] ALFALFA BREEDING 359 That the relative humidity had little to do with yield is shown by the fact that the highest averages for this factor occurred on the first and fifth cuttings which were the highest and lowest in yield, respectively. _ Although it was intended to give each plot approxi- mately the same amount of water for each cutting, uneven- ness in the slope made this impossible. The average amount of water applied to each cutting was 6.28 inches with an average standard deviation of 1.54 inches. Now, taking cognizance of this variation in the water supply, we find that its effect upon the yield was only appreciable in the fourth and fifth cuttings. Records were not made of the water supplied to the first cutting, but after that time they are complete. By reference to Table II it will be observed that these correlations in the second, third and sixth cuttings are so small as to be negligible, but in the fourth and fifth cuttings they are sufficiently large to indicate that this factor was of some importance in gov- erning the yields. These results may be interpreted as meaning that approximately 6.28 inches of water were ample for each cutting during the cooler weather of spring and fall. That too much was not given at these seasons, however, is shown by the absence of large minus correla- tions. Factors other than water supply, therefore, gov- erned the yields during these periods. Hot, dry weather came on during the growth of the third cutting, but the amount of water supplied plus the winter and spring sur- plus left in the soil was ample to mature the crop. With the continued high demand for water during the hot weather of July and August, the surplus having been exhausted and the summer rains helping but little, six and one fourth inches was not sufficient. There was, therefore, marked suffering for water, which was reflected in the yields of those plots that received slightly more or less of irrigation than the others. It would seem, therefore, that high temperature and a slight deficiency of water were the disturbing factors in 360 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vov. XLVIII the relative yields of the varieties tested, and that certain ones were more sensitive than others to these influences. If we turn to the relation between stand and yield, we shall again find a strong disturbance of the normal corre- lation as shown in the following: TABLE III CORRELATION BETWEEN STAND AND YIELD Cutting Ist 2d 3d Corrolntion 6366s + .78 + .04 + .55 + .07 + 47 + .08 Cutting 4th 5th i 6th WOrrelianion ee + 54+ .07 + .10 + .10 + .70 + .05, The exceptionally low coefficient of the fifth cutting was due to the low yields on the part of plots which had good stands but were relatively inactive during the hot weather and partial water famine which occurred at this period. On the other hand, certain plots through their resistance to heat and consequent activity at this period, overcame to a large extent their handicap of poor stands, and nearly obliterated the usual plus correlation between stand and yield. The data thus studied en masse indicate at least two physiological groups which are unequally sensitive to the climatic changes which occur in the course of a growing season, and whose reactions were sufficiently strong to change almost completely the order of the productivity of the plots. In order to test this conclusion let us turn to the individual plots and endeavor to discover and classify the physiological varieties indicated above, If, now, we arrange the forty-four regional strains according to their morphological characters and geo- graphical origin, we shall have five more or less distinct groups as follows: Mediterranean, Peruvian, European, American and Turkestan. The behavior of these varietal groups through the course of six cuttings during the sum- mer of 1910 substantiates the conclusions already drawn and illustrates the sharp differences in climatice reactions which may be observed in the several varieties of a single species. No. 570] ALFALFA BREEDING 361 Morphologically the Mediterranean and Peruvian al- falfas are so distinct in type that any one at all familiar with the different kinds of alfalfa would recognize them at a glance, whether a whole field or a single plant be observed. The presence of yellow or greenish blue flow- ers also determines a variety to be of northern origin with mixtures of falcata characters, which usually carry with them resistance to cold and drought. Otherwise, the Turkestan, American and European types are so nearly alike that only an expert would recognize them in mass culture. The individual variations within these three types intergrade to such a degree that one could scarcely assume to judge, from the observation of a single plant, the type prevailing in the field from which it originated. The three types, however, differ markedly in their phys- iological reactions as we shall presently see. The distinc- tions, in this regard, as exhibited on our plots, are not nearly so marked between the American and Turkestan alfalfas as between these two types, on the one hand, and the European, on the other. However, in northern cli- mates where winter resistance enters as a potent factor, the Turkestan alfalfa exhibits greater hardiness than the American form, and, therefore, is able to maintain a more perfect stand through seasons of extreme frost. When grown under Arizona conditions, the average yields of each of these five type groups present seasonal curves at once striking in their diversity and contrasts. These differences are exhibited more easily by plotting the average of all the plots as a straight line, and the aver- age of the different groups as percentages of the total average above and below the ‘general average line. In observing Fig.1, we are first impressed with a marked similarity in the performance of the European and Medi- terranean alfalfas, on the one hand, and the American and Turkestan on the other, and also with the striking differ- ences exhibited between the two groups.. Although the average yield of the European plots greatly exceed that of the Mediterranean plots, the shapes of their respective 362 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vow XLVIII curves are almost exactly alike, the greatest relative yield of each being in the heated part of the summer after the beginning of the water famine. In like manner, the American and Turkestan varieties made similar relative yield curves, that for the Turkestan being slightly above the curve for the American strains. Here, however, the CUTTING / 2 3 P. f é $ JF a Jt S J+ < 32 tJ > J a gar 5 at wu 2# [S] @ S t a 20 18 Me 14 12 qe f é 4 2 AVE. YIELD 27/5 LBS. 27/6 LBS 72 LBS a N + = & 5.6 As M PERCENT BELOW AVERAGE M * % ~ RELATIVE YIELD OF REGIONAL VARIETIES BASED ON THE AVERAGE OF ALL PLOTS AS 100 PER CENT. curves bend strongly downward in mid and late summer, as if these types were much less resistant to the accumula- tive effects of drought and heat. In fact, it would seem that during the hot period included within the fifth cutting No. 570] ALFALFA BREEDING 363 (July and August), the American and Turkestan varieties were comparatively inactive, yielding only about eight hundred pounds of dry hay per acre, as against more than | ton and a half each on the first cutting. The relative ~ield curve for the Peruvian type stands separate and dis- tinct from the others. Although here, as with other varie- ties, the yield declines with the advance of the season, the persistence and vigor with which this strain resisted the summer heat and drought caused it to gain rapidly on the other varieties in relative yield throughout the season until the very last cutting, when there was a slight decline. Disregarding the shape of the curves we may now notice the total yield for the season. In this respect the different regional varieties take the following relative order: Peru- vian, European, Turkestan, American and Mediterranean. _ It is here noticeable that, though the European and Medi- terranean varieties have similar seasonal yield curves, they are not contiguous in the arrangement based on total yields. This is a result of a marked difference in the stand maintained by the two varieties which averaged ninety-two per cent. for the former and seventy-four per cent. for the latter. In their ability to maintain stand, the Peruvian, European, Turkestan and American varieties were about equal, averaging 92, 92, 93 and 94 per cent., re- spectively. The lack of stand on the part of the Medi- terranean alfalfas was not due to the poor quality of the original seed, for all of these plots once had perfect stands. This behavior is also in accordance with the records of other fields of Mediterranean alfalfa in the southwest, which have come under the observation of the writer. The explanation of the weakness of the Mediterranean and corresponding strength of the otherwise similarly reacting European alfalfa in maintaining stand under Arizona con- ditions is a subject for further careful physiological study. The recognition, analysis, and calibration of these dif- ferences of the physiological reactions of varieties are thus seen to become a first essential in the study of cli- matic adaptation, and form the basis for rational pro- cedure in the choice of varieties and in selective breeding for the improvement of the same. 364 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII CORRELATIONS In the improvement of varieties of plants, quality is often as important as quantity of yield. This is especially true in a forage crop, such as alfalfa. Since nitrogen, next to fat, is the most expensive of the necessary food constit- uents, it may be taken as the measure of quality. Com- merical buyers judge alfalfa hay by its purity, odor, color and percentage of leaves retained in curing and baling. The value of the leaves lies in their relatively high nitro- gen content and the consequent increased food value which they impart to the hay. Expressed quantitatively, the cor- relations between the nitrogen content of the hay and the percentage of leaves for the six cuttings were as follows: TABLE IV CORRELATION BETWEEN NITROGEN CONTENT OF HAY AND PER CENT. OF LEAVES Cutting 1st 2a 3d Correlation yess so nae + 46+ .08 + 61+ .06 + .72 + .05 Cutting 4th 5th 6th Correlation.«..i ve.cecierss + .68 + .05 + .61 + .06 + .52 + .07. That the final value of the hay is markedly dependent upon the composition as well as the percentage of leaves is shown by the following high and fairly uniform correla- tion between the nitrogen content of the hay and the nitro- gen content of the leaves: TABLE V CORRELATION BETWEEN NITROGEN CONTENT OF Hay AND NITROGEN CONTENT F LEAVE Cutting Ist 2d 3d Cörrolation o. oic cise + .69 + .05 + .73 + .05 + .42 + .08 Cutting 4th 5th 6th Correlation, 6. ssccednacss + .67 + .06 + .85 + .03 + .74 + .05. If, now, we have shown that the quality of the hay de- pends primarily upon the percentage and composition of the leaves, we may proceed to investigate those factors which indirectly modify the feeding value by influencing the amount or character of these organs. The factors most profoundly affecting the percentage of leaves were yield, height and stage of maturity at which the cutting was made. Local or varietal forces were No. 570] ALFALFA BREEDING 365 sufficiently constant to hold the place variation of this character to the plus side of the equation for four out of five determinations made, as is seen in the following table: TABLE VI AON VARIATION IN PERCENTAGE OF _— Cortelntion. aa 10. 10 + 23 4 10 + 36. +10 + res 08 — he 10. These correlations, however, are low and seem to indicate that the natural varietal traits were being overcome and obscured by other variable factors. Contrary to expectation, the stand had little to do with the percentage of leaves, as the following low and incon- stant correlations show. TABLE VII CORRELATION BETWEEN THE PERCENTAGE OF LEAVES AND STAND Cutting 1st 2d 3d Carton saras irri — .14+.10 — .02 + .10 + .03 + .10 Cutting 4th 5th 6th Uorrelation ciso ccs +.10+.10 + .07 + .10 + .24 + .10. On the other hand, the relation between height and yield and percentage of leaves was constant and marked, except in the last two cuttings. TABLE VIII CORRELATION BETWEEN PERCENTAGE OF LEAVES AND HEIGHT AND YIELD Cutti 1st 2A 3a Yield” Pecks cae saw eh oe lie — 41 + .08 — .60 + .07 —.15+.10 Har oa a — .48 + .08 — .62 + .06 — 68 + .05 Cutti 4th 5th 6th Yield” bev esa wet oles dae — .40 + .09 + .20+.10 + .30 + .09, OIRO ce ae — 55 + 07 + .09 + .10 + .19 + .10, The sudden change from minus to plus in these correla- tions should be noted. The average heights of the first four cuttings were 32, 30, 28 and 27 inches, respectively. The average height of the fifth and sixth, were 15 and 12 inches. This would suggest that at or below 15 inches the mutual shading of the stems is not sufficient to cause an appreciable shedding of the lower leaves. Up to this point, moreover, growth usually takes place by an increase 366 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vowu. XLVIII in the number of nodes, each with its accompanying leaves and side branches. Above fifteen inches, however, the principal growth in height consists in a lengthening of the internodes and, consequently, a relatively greater produc- tion of stem as compared with leaf tissue. In this phys- iological correlation lies the core of the difficulty in breed- ing at once for quality and quantity. The act of high pro- duction within itself cuts down the quality of the product by reducing the ratio between the leaves and the stems. This difficulty, moreover, occurs in the composition as well as the percentage of the leaves. The correlation ex- isting between the nitrogen content of the leaves and the number of days required to mature a cutting is shown in the following table: TABLE IX CORRELATION BETWEEN THE NITROGEN CONTENT OF HAY AND THE PERIOD REQUIRED FOR MATURITY Cutting 1st 2d 3d Correlation: -ofo. 6 sence bes — .33 + .09 — .30 + .09 — 27 + .09 Cutting 4th 5th 6th Gorritin. vee — 52+ .07 — .50 + .08 — 17 + .10. Quickly maturing varieties thus have leaves richer in nitrogen than those which require a greater length of time for completion of growth. When, however, we take the average number of days required throughout the season to mature a cutting for each plot and compare this with the total seasonal yield we find a correlation of -+ .43. Thus we are again confronted by a minus correlation be- tween quality and yield which must be overcome if we would make progress simultaneously in both lines. As further examples of antagonistic correlations, a few instances may be taken from the data furnished by forty- three plots of pure races of alfalfa grown during the sum- mer of 1910. The correlation between height and percent- age of leaves was again constant and marked. The results here paralleled those found for the regional varieties. Whereas yield was uniformly correlated positively with both stooling capacity (av. No. stems per plant) and height, it is interesting to note that there was also a uni- No. 570] ALFALFA BREEDING 367 TABLE X CORRELATION IN PURE RACES Cuttings Correlation Between July August September | October Green weight and average | mber stems + .75 + .04/+ .42 + .08/+ .62 + .06 + .50 + .08 Green weight and average | height + .01 + 10/4 .44 + .08/+ .22 + .10 + .33 + .09 Average height and number of | ste — .29 + .09 — .19 + .10/— .82 + .09|— .21 + .10 Average height and per cent. | (NOW naea e eaa — .39 + .09|— .15 + .10/— .55 + .07 — .51 + .08 form minus correlation existing between them. We thus have two factors both making for yield, but seemingly (probably physiologically) antagonistic to each other. In breeding for high yielding strains we are here again called upon to overcome by selection an antagonistic physiolo- gical correlation. This brings us to the following final conclusion which the writer wishes to emphasize: In economic plant breeding one frequently encounters physiologically negative correlations such as those, in alfalfa, between height and stooling capacity, height and percentage of leaves, and between yield and quality. In seeking improvement, therefore, the breeder must recog- nize and make use of these facts in the interpretation of results obtained, and also search for races which violate such naturally antagonistic correlations to the greatest possible extent. GENERAL CONCLUSIONS That the complex of allelomorphs, which we call a va- riety, may be definite as both to ultimate composition and organization is not here questioned. When, however, we consider that visible characters are only the expression of the reactions of the vital forces of the plant with the en- vironment, we can realize that the variety, as we see it, is not a definite thing, but is a result of two independent classes of factors. Change either and the result corre- spondingly changes. 368 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII We are therefore to look upon the variety as a delicately organized chemical compound. The various factors of climate and soil may be compared to different physical influences to which the original compound may be sub- jected. As the chemist would expect reactions varying in accordance with the physical stimuli used, so will the plant react in agreement with the different environmental com- binations. The extent to which this will change the nature and appearance of plants is often far reaching. Cook, working with cotton, has found that certain cultural condi- tions at an early stage of growth will make profound dif- ferences in the method of branching which determines the whole subsequent development of the plant and affects materially its economic value. Cultural and climatic reac- tions often lead to error among those who assume them to be mutative changes induced by the new conditions. That these reactions may bring to light sub-races with heredi- tary tendencies not hitherto called into expression and which, by selection, may be secured as pure races, is the probable explanation of many cases of supposed direct climatic adaptation. Thus, realizing the true nature of a variety, we can draw further upon the analogy of the chemist who investi- gates an unknown substance by testing its reactions with a large number of known reagents. In like manner the breeder can only understand the true nature of the hered- itary vital forces within a plant after he has tested and calibrated its reactions against a variety of soil and cli- matic factors. These reactions are of interest to the farmer only in so far as they affect the economic value of the variety as grown in his own locality but to the breeder and student of heredity their importance is fundamental. This is so because they enable him to classify, coordinate and interpret the experimental results that he obtains. This ability finally must form the basis of all rational pro- cedure, whether one be engaged in the study of pure gen- etics or in the operations of practical plant improvement. TAXONOMY AND EVOLUTION By X, “Some passages in this book, if taken alone and read hastily, may appear to discourage systematic Zoology. This is far from my inten- tion. No one can study the great naturalists of the seventeenth and eighteenth centuries without feeling how seriously their work is impaired by the defeetive systems of the time. It is not systematic but aimless work that I deprecate—work that springs from no real curiosity in Nature and attempts to answer no scientific iad "—T. C. Miall, “Natural History of Aquatic Insects,” Preface, p. i. INTRODUCTION Linn&zvus bestowing Latin names upon animals and plants was simply tripping gaily across the back of a half submerged Behemoth and mistaking it for dry land. Now the beast is careering around, and in spite of zoological congresses and inter- national rules nobody quite knows what to do with him. No doubt when some zoological ezar arises and issues his fiat a uni- form system of nomenclature will be adopted and things will begin to straighten themselves out. This can only be a matter of time—the past can not be altered. On systematists to-day necessarily devolves the dull, difficult and important duty of going through the descriptive work of the early naturalists and emending it; so that Spallanzani’s derisive sobriquet of ‘nomenclature naturalists’’ was a little unjust, even in his time. Whatever opinions may be held upon the genius of Linneus, in justice to him it should be said that it was not until his ex- ample had been followed by a crowd of other workers eager to attain to immortality by way of the back door he had left open that the fat was really in the fire. Well knowing the confusion into which systematic work in zoology was brought by the early naturalists, modern systemat- ists in our opinion will be the authors of a similar confusion in the future if some of the slipshod methods of modern syste- matics are not corrected. Moreover, a confused nomenclature is not the least of the evils which second-rate systematic work brings in its train. : 369 370 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII Systematists with a proud curl of the lip may tell us that the work is not done now as it once was. Indeed, to those who are not able to project themselves into the future it may seem in- credible that the systematists of a later date will be able to find much room for complaint in the elaborate descriptions and care- ful figures of modern descriptive writers. For the moment, how- ever, it suffices us to point the parable by remarking that in 1780 Spallanzani was able to refer to the ‘‘beautiful figures’’ and ‘‘careful descriptions’’ of a systematic worker on frogs. We, of course, know without seeing them that the figures were not beautiful nor the description, careful—any way in the sense of being complete. We have therefore to reflect whether the zoologists of a future generation will find the work of to-day any freer of faults than that of the past centuries. vik SYSTEMATIC Work. GENERAL CONSIDERATIONS It is necessary to insist at once that systematic work is not merely a question of nomenclature, names and novelties. Sys- tematists have only themselves to thank if such a narrow con- ception of their provinee is very widely spread, especially among morphologists and anatomists, who. are ready to belittle the value of the systematists’ work. But science is measurement and zoology—if you like—is description, and it is impossible to dispense with the systematists’ descriptive work. But we think it possible to dispense with a good ‘deal of stuff after this fashion : Metopidium high, mab raktinernin rather long, acute, arcuate and curved at the tips. Pronotum roughly punctured at the bottom of fine furrows. Color dark-ochreous. Posterior horn uniformly cylindrical, undulating or sinuous without rugosities. Underside, seutellum and legs sordid-ochreous. The phrase ‘‘sordid ochreous”? comes ready to hand and ry it unnecessary for us to go in search of a suitable com- men This is the 30th memoir” writes a PPPE ‘on the Zonitidæ which I have published in this journal, describing in all about 560 new species.’’ We feel inclined to put our hands resolutely on his shoulders and inquire if he ever saw a cteno- phor swimming in thé sea or watched the progress of an Asterias towards its prey. i No: 570] “°° TAXONOMY. AND. EVOLUTION. STE: - No one'can look unmoved upon the Hymenopteran or Helicoid Specialist with head bent over a drawer full of shells or dried insects on`pins. It is not that we resent concentration or enthu- siasm or even specialization, but the systematist has lost touch with his own science of zoology. ' Zoology, a cornucopia of marvels, lies at his elbow full to over- flowing, but he is unmindful of it. It-is as if a man should use the Parthenon only ‘as a convenient place on which to strike a match for his pipe. The divorce between systematic work and the rest of zoology is the more regrettable because it is practically complete. It is, we admit, expedient that zoology should be divided up into anatomy, morphology’ and so.on. But such a division is allow- able only when it is expedient, while for intellectual purposes such a division is and has always been a danger. To obtain facts one must be an analyst, to consider them one must be a synthesist. _ Between the two there is all the difference between a hodman and a natural philosopher. - But our contention is that not even the plea of practical ex- Pia ior can justify the extreme state of specialization into which: systematie zoology has fallen, making itself manifest in the concatenation of such purely artificial characters as that ‘the third joint of the antenna is longer than the second, that the mesoscutellum ‘is ovate and the color pink with blue spots.’’ All this simply makes one yawn, though there is this much to be said in favor! of this stamp of systematist, that nothing bores him so much as the recitation of one of his own diagnoses or being introduced to the systematist of another group. Systematic work is a withered branch of the biological tree which’ there is still hope of rejuvenating. Treviranus long ago remarked that if we once regarded systematic. work as a part of biology and nomenclature as a means to an end rather than as an end in itself, both might take their places in science. Let us take every precaution against systematic work becoming one of those unproductive and artificial pursuits which spring up like mushrooms around centers of splendid endeavor and high achievement. After Shakespeare came his commentators. Shall it be said that after biology came the systematists? ^. We assume that the principal object of systematic work is to discover the phylo-genetic. classification of animals, for which it is surely necessary that every animal as it passes through the 372 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vow. XLVIII systematists’ hands should be, as far as possible, thoroughly ex- amined and described, no dependence being placed upon a few superficial characters usually selected from the external parts? That the systematist should concern himself, as he does, with the external parts, leaving the anatomy to other workers, we consider is as bad for the systematist himself as it is bad for the science; for himself, he is doing work which can only keep his soul alive with difficulty—superficial clerical work which can be ‘prompted by no real curiosity and attempts to answer no scientific questions,’’ and the results of the work itself is often invalidated by the arrival of the destroying angel in the person of the anatomist. For a superficial description often means a wrong classification ; whence it follows that any zoo-geographical deductions therefrom are invalidated; while a careless descrip- tion usually ignores the possibilities of variation and shows no evidence of pains having been taken to make identification easy. Systematic work, then, is concerned with classification, geo- graphical distribution, variation and identification, and there would be no need for this paper, if it were more generally re- alized that one thorough examination and description of the whole animal assists those branches of the inquiry more than twenty loose and superficial ones. Of course systematic workers are not the only zoologists who over-publish ; yet they especially might cultivate a little of the salutary reticence of C. L. Nitsch and Alfred Newton, who, with no discredit to themselves, wrote and published little, yet it must be admitted by those with an eye on the extravagant output of others, to the advantage of zoology. The words ‘‘res non-verba’’ were the motto of Delle Chiaje, who, like Nitzsch, on his death left behind many important discoveries unpublished and only indicated in his drawings. CLASSIFICATION IN GENERAL The coming of Evolution meant for systematic workers that no system of classification would henceforth be considered as a serious contribution to science, which was not constructed on phylogenetic lines. It meant the final overthrow of such ideas as Agassiz held, that the divisions of the animal kingdom were instituted by the Divine Intelligence as categories of his mode of thought—of such fantastic systems as those, of Rafinesque and Swainson and such strictly artificial ones as the arbitrary ar- No. 570] TAXONOMY AND EVOLUTION. 373 rangements of convenience which should be now used only in those groups where, and for as long as, our knowledge of the anatomy is so slight that some sort of temporary device for sorting out genera and species has to be adopted. The ideal system is now phylogenetic, i. e., it aims at recon- structing in a genealogical tree the actual lines of descent. Only those who have attempted the reconstruction of phylo- genetic trees understand the intrinsic difficulties of the work. There can be no doubt that the coming of Evolution has put before the systematist a very difficult task, As to whether the methods usually employed by him are adequate to the demands placed upon them we are frankly sceptical. Fortunately for the systematist the main lines of classifica- tion in most groups are given him ready made by the morphol- ogists who have laid down the foundations trusting to the ‘‘sys- tematist’’ to fill in the details. Such classifications—the main phyla, classes and orders are of permanent value, because they are founded upon a combination of characters of tried worth judiciously selected after a careful survey of extensive embry- ological and anatomical data. SINGLE CHARACTER CLASSIFICATION On the other hand the minor systems—the families, genera and species—the realm of the ‘‘systematist’’—too frequently consist of haphazard combination of a few characters selected because of their convenience in not entailing any anatomical work, or selected on account of the ignorance existing of any other—particularly internal—important characters. Ignorance of their morphology has been the main reason for the difficulty in classifying the Coleoptera. Entomologists are especially prone to give their whole attention to what is visible without the aid of dissection. In the Polyzoa the majority of forms are only known by their external appearance and their classification is proportionally unsatisfactory. In the Mollusca reliance is placed on the shell; in mammals the skull and the skin, in birds the plumage are the articles of faith. Single character classification or diagnosis by one or two characters, as zoological history shows, has proved inadequate— that it is unphilosophical is patent to all. Such single character classification even when practised by 374 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII the great morphologists, men who, being acquainted with the whole of the anatomy of the forms they were classifying, de- liberately selected one or two characters after a survey of the whole—was rarely a success. Huxley set out unabashed to classify birds by their palate, and Agassiz fish by their scales— systems which have now shared the fate of most others which set out to erect a classification on the modifications of a single organ alone. Alfred Newton said that there was no part of a bird’s organization that by a proper study would not help to settle the great question of its affinities. The systematist who deals with the minor subdivisions of the animal kingdom—families and genera—shou e as much a morphologist as the one who deals with the larger—the phyla and classes. DESCRIPTION We have pointed out above that the adequacy of a system of classification depends in great measure upon the thoroughness of the description of the species and genera. Classification in all groups has progressed in just proportion to the more exact examination of the species considered in the classification. | The history of zoological research brings out this fact very clearly, beginning with the work of Linnzus, the originator of the superficial diagnosis, passing on through Cuvier, who appre- ciated the value of anatomical knowledge, to Von Baer, who emphasized the importance of embryology. _ It was not a “‘systematist’’ as we know him who first correctly classified Lepas—the econchologists blindly accepted it as a Molluse. It was not a ‘‘systematist’’ who first established Peri- patus as an Arthropod, for the first describer of that animal regarded it as a slug! How rare it is to find in a description of a new species any- thing more than an indication of the external parts. It is a peculiarly arbitrary limit to a man’s curiosity that restricts his enquiry to the superficial aspect of an animal. A natural philosopher ought never to be satisfied with the external ap- pearance of things. The wisdom of the ancients bids us ‘‘be- ware of what things appear ’’; and the method of our modern science is one of close and detailed observations. In scattering names broadcast with liberal largesse upon species, varieties and No. 570] TAXONOMY AND EVOLUTION. 375 genera, systematists have sometimes dropped into some curious errors. Teratological specimens have been described as new species and most zoologists have heard of the man who de- scribed as a new species the longicorn beetle, the head of which having fallen off, had been fixed on upside down. His examina- tion of a new species makes so slight an impression on his mind that sometimes the same worker has described the same form twice under different names. The descriptive papers on Mollusca usually consist of short descriptions of the shells, even written in a dead language. This is conchology. Conchologists confine themselves to the pat- terns and shapes of shells—nature’s medallions—numismatics ! Much of this work—along with similar productions in entomol- ogy and carcinology—we regard as positively fiagitious. Sir Ray Lankester in the article ‘‘Zoology’’ in the Encyclo- pedia Britannica (ed. XI.) remarks that museum. naturalists must give attention to the inside as well as to the outside of animals and that to-day no one considers a study of an animal’s form of any value which does not include internal structure, histology and embryology in its scope. Agassiz, too in his famous ‘‘Essay on classification’’ wrote that ‘‘the mere indi- cation of a species is a poor addition to our knowledge when compared with such monographs as Lyonnet’s Cossus, Bojanus’ ‘Turtle’ Strauss Durekheim’s Melolontha and Owen’s Nauti- lus.” i ‘“‘But,’’ it will immediately be asked in chorus, ‘‘do you seriously suggest that a monographie volume should be devoted to every new species?’’ This is a leading question which brings us to the crux of the whole matter, and ean not be answered in simple ‘‘Yea’’ or Ney?’ . THE PROVISIONAL DIAGNOSIS The amount. of analytical study that may be given to any one animal form in any one stage of its development is infinite. The result is that in describing a new species for the purposes of exact phylogenetic classification there must be a limit beyond which it is unnecessary to go. Such a limit can not be otherwise than arbitrarily selected according to the best judgment of the systematic worker as to how much analysis is required to place his new species, although at present, miserabile dictu, relatively * 376 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vov. XLVIII very few animals have been thoroughly explored, yet in the dis- tant future, in the millennium, it can not be doubted that every genus, even every species will have been examined in toto in every stage of its development and life-history as thoroughly as our instruments and eyesight will allow, and perhaps a whole vol- ume or several volumes will be devoted to every animal form. At present, however, it is a waste of ink to consider a future so far away. A more pressing duty is to consider how far modern methods of superficial diagnosis fulfil the obligations placed upon systematists not to give an exhaustive analysis of animal forms, but to give sufficient data to meet the searching demands of phylogenetic classification. We are aware of the fact that the convinced and determined systematist does not maintain that the method of superficial diagnosis does meet or is intended to meet the demands we have been indicating. If he reads as far as this and does not throw aside this paper in contempt, he is ready with eager forefinger and glib apology to convict us of begging the question that sys- tematic zoology can be ever anything, or should be ever any- thing more than we have sai It is often argued that the srperiéial diagnosis of the syste- matic worker is simply a provisional diagnosis awaiting the con- firmation of the anatomist. A plausible defence of the provi- sional diagnosis is advanced by many workers in perfect good faith which it is now necessary to anticipate and examine. This argument defends the provisional diagnosis on two grounds: (1) The advertisement theory; (2) the recognition mark theory. The supporters of these theories admit that the provisional diagnosis in no way settles either an animal’s systematic posi- tion or its validity as a species. But it is alleged to be of value and should be encouraged because it advertises the existence of a presumptive new form which would otherwise remain un- known and overlooked in the store rooms of the museum and laboratory, and because in giving an account of the external parts, at all events, the systematist is describing those features by which we are more or less easily able by a superficial exami- nation to recognize summarily the form when it turns up again. The first part of our answer amounts to a recapitulation of what has been previously stated in general, viz., that systematies have lost touch with the rest of the science. The output of systematic No. 570] TAXONOMY AND EVOLUTION. 377 work and the output of anatomical and morphological work nowadays move along completely different channels. The work turned out by the systematic worker is scarcely, if ever, con- ceived in the light of modern biological theory, is rarely couch in terms of modern biology and rarely indicates a problem to be solved or a question to be answered. It proposes distinctions the anatomist sweeps away and hazards affinities the morphol- ogist laughs at. It performs work that has to be done over again, and instead of giving the morphologist what it claims to give him—a sketch map of the country he is to traverse—all it does is to bewilder him with a Will-of-the-Wisp’s lantern, an intolerable multitude of slipshod and untrustworthy directions that he has come instinctively to suspect. We can not too often ask the question, why should the work be done twice? Surely it is time that something were done to stop this tremendous rush for publishing provisional diagnoses that more time could be devoted to the systematic study of animal forms, obtaining thereby sound phylogenetic classification, sound deductions in geographical distribution, valid species and a less confused nomenclature. Thus the systematist’s protest that at least he ‘‘advertises’’ pre- sumptive new forms we can reply that he may do so, but that for any purpose other than a dull census of the animal kingdom with a very generous ‘‘-+’’ to it, me is a positive Benedick of zoolo- gists, for ‘‘nobody miris him.’ The upholders of the provisional diagnosis will say that at any rate they are giving us a description of the external parts and are increasing our knowledge by so much. True, but by so inconsiderable an amount that when the anatomist comes along with his scalpel he so quickly disposes of the external parts merely by the use of his eyes that it is a matter of indifference whether the former have been described or not. Moreover, the great majority of the tens of thousands of descriptions that are issuing from the press are of animals so closely related to pre- viously described species that such descriptions really amount to little more than a recitation of their distinguishing characters. It is certainly useful to know that Caccabis rufa is to be dis- tinguished from Perdix cinerea by its red legs and that the Leporide can be discriminated by the character of their upper incisors. But the question may well be asked, what is the use of being able to distinguish one species from another without 378 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII being able to record at the same time anything about its bionom- ies or anatomy which would give the distinction its real value. A great deal is known about the partridges and hares, hence the distinctions alluded to above are useful as an easy way of quickly identifying them. But so long as nothing is known about either of two species that are distinguished we are none the worse off, if both remain indistinguishable. Finally we would point out that of all people the systematist should know that at present of the forms he advertises and describes so copiously and summarily only a fractional part is, or can be, dealt with by the laboratory worker. We are speaking now of the anatomy pure and simple of new species and genera. The laboratory worker proceeds slowly, is fewer in numbers and has other problems—embryology (descriptive and experi- mental), heredity, physiology (descriptive and experimental) and morphology to attend to besides purely descriptive anatomy. And yet anatomy—the very corner stone of the temple of zoology—has to be restricted in output because none of the sys- tematists will learn how to use a scalpel or look down a dissect- ing-microscope—feats in themselves perfectly easy and calling for no special training or faculties. - Possibly the upholders of the provisional diagnosis will main- tain that by publishing his account of the difference between closely allied forms the systematist is providing the biologist with a stimulus to discover how much deeper such differences go. But surely it is a strange perversion of a man’s natural instinct of curiosity that enables the systematist to rest content with advertising problems instead of endeavoring to equip himself for the task of undertaking them himself, who is eminently suited to the work and whose — daily brings him into close contact with them. Finally we would point out that the enormous mass of species which have been created upon superficial diagnosis so far have remained unincorporated for the most part in the structure it is designed to build up, viz., a clear comprehension of the phylogeny of the lesser divisions of the animal kingdom. It is as though a man were to set about building a house by making a vast quantity of bad bricks and then to leave them scattered about his site in the hopes that some one would come along and make a house of them. Surely it is an economy of effort for the systematist to take up the bricks and build himself, what time No.570] `. TAXONOMY AND EVOLUTION: 379 the T and morphologist are engaged upon their own - special tasks. THE COMPARATIVE VALUE OF INTERNAL AND EXTERNAL PARTS Briefiy reviewing the discussion as far as we have carried it, it will be seen that we are asking for sound phylogenetic classi- fication of the smaller groups as well as of the larger ones, based not upon single characters, but upon the whole of the characters regarded collectively, for more careful and more thorough mor- phological methods in description and for. the discontinuation of the provisional diagnosis. In view of the desirability of work- ing up sounder schemes of classification from the enormous, un- wieldy and superficially known mass of genera and species sys- tematists can be rendering little service by continuing to turn out indiscriminate provisional diagnoses. It remains now to discuss in greater detail the proposal we bring forward in the place of the provisional diagnosis. The commonly accepted opinion is that while for the classifi- cation of families and orders the internal parts must be taken into consideration, for that of species and genera a summary of the external parts is all that is required. On account of the labor and difficulty sometimes involved in dissection we are too ready to assume that the internal parts in genera and species present a dismal monotomy of character which it would be profitless to investigate for systematic purposes. If it is admitted that internal characters are of value among the higher divisions of the animal kingdom, can the systematist tell us at what precise point in the downward scale they cease to have value, and at which reference need only be made to the external parts? Even supposing for a moment that there is such a limit, we are strongly of opinion that it does not come before the genera. A genus is of different value in different groups but as a rule it presents so much difference in external form from other genera as to warrant the inference that internal differences of a like extent will be found if sought for. At the present moment a genus is a perfectly arbitrary collection of species. We ven- ture to prophesy that with more elaborate descriptions inter- generic relationships will be more carefully defined and genera will become less heterogeneous and more natural. But this is by the way. 380 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST — [Vou. XLVIII A priori it seems improbable that less variety will be found among the various internal systems of organs than in the in- tegumentary or exoskeletal parts. But an argument may be put forward that the external parts in immediate contact with the environmental forces would be the first to register change in the modification of a species. The internal parts as stanchions and bulwarks remain firm to give characters to orders and fami- lies, while change makes assault without and gives characters for species. For example, among the Asteroids it is said that the internal organization is so uniform that the only method of classification is to take the different ways in which the demands of the external environment have been met. : But generally speaking a species depends for its survival not simply upon the external front it presents to its environment. An animal’s form cannot arbitrarily be divided into external and internal parts. It is an integral whole, and variation and selection may occur anywhere, while the correlation of variation is a text-book commonplace. As opposed to correlative variation there is the law of the independent variation of parts. Not only may variation occurring in one part cause a variation to take place in another, but variation may take place independently in some areas and be limited in another, so that in deciding upon the comparative value of the internal and external parts in any group consideration must be given to both these laws. In the Asteroids, we assume that anatomists have taken the matter in hand and found that the external parts vary as a rule independ- ently of the internal which remain constant. But in how few groups has such a precaution been taken! Is it not rather the general rule simply to assume that the internal parts lack varia- tion and are of no value systematically, as, for instance, in the Lepidoptera, where the Lepidopterists expect that a classification based upon the wing-markings or upon wing-neuration can ex- press the true relationship of the various units? Even in those groups where systematists have dissected and found the internal parts valueless it still remains necessary, in view of the law of independent and unexpected variation of parts for them, to apply the scalpel to every new form It is impossible to deny that the external parts are often of extreme systematic importance—they are exposed to the light and develop color patterns (although color is usually an unsafe guide if taken alone), and the external parts of such forms as No. 570] TAXONOMY AND EVOLUTION. 381 Arthropods and Molluses being hard provide systematists with a sculpture on which it is easy to detect minute differences in pattern. On the other hand we would remind the conchologist that the external parts are by their very positions most liable to exhibit lesions and weathering, and certainly in the case of Mollusca where the dependence of the exoskeleton upon a spe- cific article of diet (viz., lime salts) is very close, to register “*fluctuating variation” according to the constitution of the medium or of the food ingested. But here again if a more common practise were made in dis- secting by systematists, variations would be found even in closely allied species making the descriptions complete and often even necessitating the erection of new genera. One of the writers was dissecting an ordinary species when he discovered that the epipharynx was so entirely different in form and struc- ture from the usual type for the genus that, had it been an external character it would long ago have been formed into a new genus. Karel Thon? has demonstrated how in Holothyride a single internal structure is at variance with the other indications of genetic affinity. A great many similar instances will be immedi- ately called to mind by those who practise dissection. Again, if systematists are convinced of the taxonomic value of hard parts how comes it that they. need to be reminded that there are hard parts in the internal anatomy as well which they so frequently and habitually leave unnoticed? The endoskele- ton of Arthropods, gastric mills, pharyngeal ossicles and carti- laginous supports are all systems which might be profitably studied by the entomologist and carcinologist, while the con- chologist generally proceeds as though the radula and jaw were part of the ‘‘mush,’’ as he so inelegantly terms the viscera. GEOGRAPHICAL DISTRIBUTION The advent of the morphologist into the particular sphere of systematics or the metamorphosis of the systematist into a mor- phologist (it matters not how we put this desirable event) will result in the annexation not only of classification, but also of questions of geographical distribution by anatomy and morphol- How many pretty theories in geographical distribution 1 Zool. Iahb., Bd, XXIII, Syst., pp. 720-21. 382 THE. AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVHE have éollapsed because they were built on the sands ofan in- correct classification? The similarity between the faunas of South America and Madagascar is ‘supported. by many- facts, but the value of Solénodon in Cuba and Centetes in Madagascar has been lesseried by the recognition that the two genera re- semble each other by convergence, and should now be paatna in different families. The Dendrobatine also are’ rébuekdersd by Dr. Gadow as. an unnatural group, the two divisions—South American and Mas- earerie—having, according to him, lost their teeth independently. Again, Dr. Gadow refers to the Ratite as a heterogeneous as- semblage of birds which is ‘‘absolutely’ worthless’? for -the zoogeographer. There are scores of such artificial groupings— the work of the a vette ii rahi wa APOT astray. The result is ENI ayetematid wolk: as at aol fiend is of very little use to us in the study of geographical distribution. It is hopeless nowadays for a zoologist to sit down with “a list of species and their range ‘and trusting implicitly in sys- tematic work to make maps of-distribution’ and, as he so often does, to draw deductions therefrom, for the validity of such de- ductions must ultimately depend upon the anatomical and mor- phological data. Moreover the study of geographical distribu- tion is developing new methods of tackling its problems. ` We do not consider it necessary tó touch on the other remedies that might be applied with a view to redeeming zoological taxo- nomy from its present artificial state A to oe it into line with the rest of biology Such remedies—for fetannd testing the validity “of species by genetic experiment and the intensive study of variation— have been advocated many times before,? although with little success. We believe, however, that the reforms in descriptive zoology we have advocated above are the more urgent. ' 2Cf. E. B. Poulton, ‘‘Essays on Evolution,’? 2. ‘*What.is a-Species?’’ ` and K. Jordan, ‘‘ Novitates Zoologice,’’ 3, 189 SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION NABOURS’S GRASSHOPPERS, MULTIPLE ALLELO-— | MORPHISM, LINKAGE AND MISLEADING TERMINOLOGIES IN GENETICS In a review of Nabours’s breeding experiments with grass- hoppers,’ Mr. Dexter makes a distinction between an interpreta- tion of Nabours’s and his own, where I fail to see a difference ex- cept in terminology. This is so typical of much recent Mendelian work that I am tempted to call attention to it. Nabours describes a cross between a female with characters BI and a male with characters CE and comments on the production of an individual with characters BEI. He says, as quoted, that the ‘‘female parent gave at least one gamete containing the fac- tors for the patterns of both her parents (B and J) and that this double character gamete was fertilized by one of the E gametes which came from the CE male.” Dexter prefers to call the supposed exceptional BI gamete of Nabours Bcel, and the supposed E sperm which fertilized it bcEi, stating that Nabours’s terminology would involve multiple allelo- morphism, his own linkage. (Nabours uses, I think, neither ex- pression.) Now what is the difference between the two interpre- tations? Is it anything but verbal? Is there anything significant in the small letters which Dexter has added to Nabours’s form- ule? If so, what is their significance? Do they mean any more than the extra zeros in the expression 1.000 as compared with 1.0? Dexter proposes an experimental test, that the cross be re- peated. “If then BEI forms should appear again and in these when mated to other forms the factors B and I should be found to stay together to the same extent as they before separated, it would show that close linkage, rather than multiple allelomorph- ism explains this particular instance.” How would it show it? If we take Nabours’s assumption that B and J have exceptionally gone into a single gamete and formed with E a zygote BIE, would it be counter to his assumption that they should subse- quently hang together and that gametes should arise BI and E, respectively? Would adding a few small letters to the formule 1 Am. Nar., May, 1914. 383 384 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII alter the case, changing it from multiple allelomorphism to link- age’? It seems to me that this is one more case in which a fallac- ious conclusion is reached in consequence of using small letters for absent characters in Mendelian formule. Professor James Wilson has pointed out others. W. E. CASTLE BUSSEY INSTITUTION, ForEsT HILLS, MASS., May 6, 1914 VOL. XLVIII, NO. 571 JULY, 1914 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST A MONTHLY JOURNAL Devoted to the Advancement of the Biological Sciences with Special Reference to the Factors of Evolution CONTENTS Page I. Pattern Development in Mammals and Birds. GLOVER M. ALLEN - — 385 II. Internal Relations of Terrestrial Associations. ARTHUR G. VESTAL- - 413 Til. Shorter Articles and Discussion: Another Hypothesis to Account for Dr. Swingle’s Experiments with Citrus. A. C. and A, L, HAGEDOORN THE SCIENCE PRESS LANCASTER, PA. GARRISON, N. ¥. NEW YORK: SUB-STATION 84 The American Naturalist intended for panais and books, etc., intended for review should be MSS sent to the Editor of THE AMER Short CAN NATURALIST, articles containing summaries of research work bear Garrison- -on-Hu rs New York, on the problems of = evolution are especially welcome, and will be given iprefeteiae in publication ndrea reprints of egika are supplied to authors free of charge. ne hu Further reprints will be supplied a ubscri paa and advertisement should be sent to the publishers. a year. Foreign postage Canadian postage twenty- -five cabs additional. subscription price is four dollar forty cents. The s fifty cents and The orge for single copies is The advertising rates are Four Dollars for a pa THE SCIENCE PRESS Lancaster, Pa. Garrison, N. Y. NEW YORK: Sub-Station 84 Entered as second-class matter, April 2, 1908, at the Post Office at Lancaster, Pa., under the Act of Congress of March 3, 1879. FOR SALE ARCTIC, ICELAND and GREENLAND BIRDS’ SKIN Well os cen Prices rticu G. DINESEN. ota jt Husavik, North Iceland, Via Leidle, England WANTED TO PURCHASE a set of BIRDS OF AMERICA by J. J. Audubon, 7 or 8 volumes, please report, stating cash price, stat- ing condition, binding and dates of volumes. F. C. HARRIS, Box 2244 Boston, Massachusetts For Sale Entire An important collection of Indian Birds’ Eggs, containing approximately 3,300 specimens of about 620 species, and in- cluding among other varieties the Green- ish Willow Warbler (Acanthopneuste viridanus), Red-browed Finch (Calla- pygius), Bearded Vulture (Gypaétus barbatus), numerous Cuckoos with their hosts, etc., ete. Particulars and full list may be had from W. F. H. ROSENBERG 57 Haverstock Hill London, N. W. Photographic Films Developed 5 cents per Spool, Prints 3 cents up, Post Cards 5 cents. 8x10 Enlargements 25 cents mounted. or Samples and Price List. FRANK R. BARBEAU 89 West Bridge Street Oswego, N. Y- eae E The iray of Chicago e set eng Saw à during t Erga quarters of the academic th 4 The cee Bs evades colleges, the A graduat ools, and the [aeg ua! sional schools provide courses 10 er Qu 2d Term July 23-Aus- 28 pas cannes will be pon application. The University of Chicago (Mitchell Tower Chicago, Minois = THE AMERICAN NATURALIST Vor. XLVIII July, 1914 No. 571 PATTERN DEVELOPMENT IN MAMMALS AND BIRDS GLOVER M. ALLEN, Boston SOCIETY or NATURAL HISTORY THe particular coloring of mammals and birds is pro- duced by two factors—pigmentation and the physical structure of the hair or feathers. Both are often present together. In certain mammals, for example the golden mole (Chrysochloris) and the European Galemys, a beautifully iridescent sheen is produced by the reflection of light rays having a certain angle of incidence upon the hairs which themselves contain pigment of a character- istic color. In the duckbill (Ornithorhynchus) the same thing is found. The peculiarity of feather structure that causes iridescence is largely developed in certain families of birds, as the hummingbirds and the pigeons (see Strong, 1904, for an account of the feather structure). It is not my purpose to discuss the use of this irides- cence to the bird, beyond stating my belief that it is in part at least for sexual display, as no one can doubt who watches the male street pigeon strutting before his mate. With amorous coos and lowered head, he confronts her and, swelling out his throat feathers, turns about and about, so that the light is reflected from his neck and throat in a sparkle of rainbow hues. It has also been Suggested (Thayer, 1909) that iridescence may be a strong factor in concealment, since from the variety of the colors produced the bird is more difficult to resolve from its many tinted environment amid foliage and flowers. 385 386 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII With many birds the characteristic coloration may not be at all that of its pigment. Thus the blue of the male indigo bird (Passerina cyanea) is due solely to the phys- ical structure of its feathers which though pigmented with brown, appear blue by reflected light. If, however, a blue feather be immersed in oil and viewed under a microscope by transmitted light, it is seen to be brown- pigmented. The physical feather-structure of the adult male is thus in this species a secondary sexual character chiefly developed during the breeding period. The important point at present is, however, that the color effects just described are none the less due to pig- ment, quite apart from the fact that the apparent color of the pigmented area may be different from the actual color of the pigment (except that iridescence may some- times be faintly seen in an unpigmented feather). The use of pigmentation to its possessor is a matter still under discussion and investigation. In many cases it is doubtless the result of purely physical causes and it is quite without the power of the animal to make use of its coloration for outward effect. Thus the beautiful colors inside the shells of some molluscs are never appar- ent from an exterior view, and are supposed by some to be in part a waste product, the result of metabolism within the organism. The present discussion has to do only with the external pigmentation of the hair and feathers, respectively, in mammals and birds. The simplest cases of coloration are those in which the body or its covering is everywhere of the same hue, or nearly so—as in the elephant, the wild buffalo, or the house mouse in which the hairy covering (or hide in the elephant) is of a nearly uniform tone everywhere. So too, the crow, the apteryx, and the nestlings of many birds whose parents show a more highly differentiated style of markings. Such mammals and birds, so far as the development of pattern is concerned, I would con- sider unspecialized, yet it does not follow that in this No. 571] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 387 respect they are also primitive, though in most cases I venture to think this may be true. The uniformity of plumage is probably a derived condition in such a species as the Cuban blackbird (Holoquiscalus assimilis) in which the duller colored females have yet a yellow patch at the bend of the wing, a style of marking widespread among allied forms. The adult males, however, have lost this’ and are wholly black. Gadow as well as Keeler (1893) conclude that among related species in which there is a tendency to differentiation of the coloring the end result of the stages through which the species may pass is the production of a wholly black bird. In general a wholly black condition is no doubt to be considered as a derived rather than a primitive state among birds whereas a uni- formly dull plumage of a brownish or grayish tone is probably in most cases primitive. Among mammals the same is probably also true, for in both the black condi- tion indicates either an excessive production of the black over other associated pigments, or a loss of the power to produce the latter, whereas the neutral gray or brownish coloring is due to a more even mixture of such pigments. As pointed out by Professor W. E. Castle, the ‘‘ticked”? pattern of the hairs of mammals is probably primitive, and it is certainly very widespread. It is well illustrated, for example, by the house mouse (Mus musculus) or the wild guinea-pig (Cavia), in which three separate pig- ments occur as granules in the individual hairs—yellow, chocolate, and black. These three in their normal mix- - ture produce a neutral gray tint—mouse color—and an examination of this type of coat usually shows that some hairs are wholly black, others dark at base barred with black and yellowish near the tip. There are two ways in which patterns may be developed from a uniformly tinted covering of hair or feathers: (1) by a local change in the relation of the associated pig- ments so that in certain areas only one or two sorts are produced instead of three, or only one; (2) by a failure to 388 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII develop pigment at all in certain places, so that a white or unpigmented area is produced. It is not rare among mammals to find that one or more of the characteristic sorts of pigments are not produced in certain individuals and probably the factor or factors for these are lost altogether from the somatic and sex cells alike. Such variations may be perpetuated through inbreeding and so no doubt have arisen sundry domestic color varieties of animals and plants. For example, in the course of experiments with color varieties of the house mouse (carried on some years since with Professor W. E. Castle) we found that the chocolate-colored mice which we bred as extracted recessives from black mice, contained only chocolate pigment in their hair, whereas in the black parents both black and chocolate pigments were present, but the black masked a chocolate pigment. Moreover, the chocolate mice always bred true to that color, but if bred back to the black parents, gave black young or both black and chocolate in Mendelian propor- tions, according to the nature of the matings. The inter- esting point here is that the chocolate mouse once pro- duced, through the loss of its black-and-gray-pigment- potentiality, can transmit no other pigment character but the chocolate. What causes the occasional production of an individual in which one or more of the characteristic sorts of pigment is absolutely lacking is still unexplained. Nevertheless it is of frequent occurrence not only among domesticated species, in which the natural conditions of life are so greatly modified, but also in species in a state of nature. A skunk normally marked, but chocolate instead of black, a raccoon likewise of normal pattern but the pig- mented areas yellow, are merely examples of the drop- ping out of the factor for black pigment from the normal combination of the two. Such specimens are of occa- sional occurrence, and examples are in the museum of the Boston Society of Natural History. Similarly are pro- duced red woodchucks or muskrats, or wholly yellow field No. 571] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 389 mice (Microtus). Melanism commonly results through an excess of black pigment which may mask a second pigment. Thus the black hairs of ‘the black variety of fancy mouse commonly contain a considerable amount of chocolate pigment as well, and so of the hairs of the black-appearing skunk. A black mouse thus does not contain the yellow pigment, while the chocolate pigment is largely masked in general view by the black. In other cases it may be that black pigment alone is present. It is probable that many cases of dichromatism among animals are explicable as similar cases in which one or other of the pigments normally present becomes to a greater or less degree inactive. Thus red forms of certain blackish or dull-colored bats (e. g., the small Molossus of Cuba) are apparently the result of the dropping out of the factor for black pigment or its great reduction. The red and gray phases of the screech owl (Otus asio) are probably also explicable as a similar phenomenon. It is only when this inactivity of one or more of the pigment factors occurs locally on the body that a definite color pattern is produced, in which neighboring areas of the body are of contrasting hues. As an example may be cited the variegated guinea-pigs, whose monotone ances- tors are still abundant in a wild state in South America. Professor Castle, through his studies of these patterns in guinea-pigs, first suggested to me in 1903 that there were definite areas of the body which, though contiguous, are independent of each other in their pigment-producing capacity. In this suggestion lies the key to the chief investigation of this paper, namely, the defining of these areas, and a study of their behavior in the development of pattern by the second of the two methods previously given—that is, through the failure of pigment to develop, so that white or colorless areas result. This condition of partial albinism is not uncommon among animals which in their normal condition are completely pigmented. In domestic species it is very general and in them tends to be preserved. It also occurs normally in the shape of defi- 390 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVI nite white markings in the patterns of many mammals and birds. Magazines of natural history abound with instances of total or of partial albinism among mammals and birds, either of domesticated or of wild species. Some writers have even recognized the fact that such white markings tend to occur in certain parts of the body, as at the tip of the tail or on the forehead. Darwin speaks of the white forehead spot or star, and the white feet so common among horses, and implies that such markings must be of some significance. His statement on hearsay that white-marked horses are more suscep- tible to poisoning from noxious herbs is, however, un- corroborated. In 1882, W. H. Brewer gathered a number of statistics as to the presence of white marks in horses and cows, but reached no conclusion. He could find no necessary correlation between the presence or absence of white spots in forehead and feet, though it appeared that white marks might be more frequent on one side of the body than the other. But the tentative conclusion that such animals habitually reclined on the side showing the more white, is begging the question. As briefly stated in my paper of 1904, the important thing is not that white tends to appear at certain places, but the converse, that pigment production is more intense at certain definite centers on the body and the occurrence of white or pigmentless areas is due to the restriction of pigment formation at the periphery of these centers, so that white occurs at their extremities or as breaks be- tween contiguous color patches. In mammals and birds these centers are typically five on each side of the body, and a median one on the fore- head. They appear to be homologous in both groups, though in different species they show varying degrees of modification in their behavior and development. When a reduction of the pigment areas occurs, the appearance is as it were a shrinking of the particular color patch toward its definite center. The reduction may vary to any degree, from that condition in which the break No. 571] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 391 between two adjacent patches is merely indicated by a white streak to that in which it is reduced to a small spot of pigment, or to zero, when the entire patch drops out, leaving a white area. These patches are wholly independ- ent of each other in the extent to which they may be developed, so that a particular patch may be quite want- ing on one side of the body, while its fellow of the oppo- site side is completely developed. Nevertheless, there is often a marked tendency to bilateral symmetry in such reduction. From a study of partial albinos in which the pigment reduction is considerable, the location of the ultimate centers of these patches becomes possible as well as the determination of their normal extent. I have studied several domesticated species in which white marks are common, with the results briefly detailed below. When all the centers are fully developed the animal is completely pigmented; when none is developed, it is a total albino. Between these extremes may be found every conceivable degree of development. In an ideal case in which each center is slightly reduced so as to be circum- scribed by white, the animal would have a dark coronal or crown patch and a series of five patches on each side separated by a median dorsal and a median ventral stripe. The anteriormost of the lateral patches center at the base of each ear, and each in its greatest development covers the side of the head from muzzle to behind the ear. These I have called the aural or ear patches; the next posterior are the two neck or nuchal patches each of which pigments its proper side of the neck, and extends from behind the ear to the shoulder and anterior edge of the foreleg. When much reduced the patch, as it were, contracts to a small area on each side of the neck, varying slightly in its location among different species. Posterior to these come the scapular or shoulder patches one on each side of the body. Each pigments the shoulder area and foreleg, except (usually) the front edge of the upper part of that member. This patch shows interesting slight variations in the extent over which it spreads in different 392 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Von. XLVIII species. Centering nearly at the lower part of the back are the pleural or side patches, each of which pigments the area from the shoulder to the lumbar region and ante- rior part of the hind leg of either side. Last of all, the two sacral or rump patches, each of which on its respect- ive side pigments the buttocks and tail. In most species these two patches are so closely associated that they tend to remain fused dorsomedially, so as to give the appear- ance, when reduced, of a single median patch at the base of the tail. Their frequent bilaterality, however, indi- cates the dual origin of such median patches. Each of the lateral patches in its complete development extends from the mid-dorsal to the mid-ventral line or those of opposite sides may overlap slightly. Reduction usually first appears mid-ventrally. It is probable that the retinas should also be considered as an additional pair of patches, since morphologically the eye is of dermal origin, and there is sometimes seen a tendency to the formation of a small cireumorbital patch, which appears to break from the ear patch when this is largely reduced. A Pocock (1907) has pointed out that in black-and-tan dogs the tan appears about the muzzle, along the sides and on the limbs, while the blacker portions are more dorsal. It may be added that in tricolor hounds, in which the several primary patches are reduced, these are often tan color at their several peripheries and black centrally. In both cases, the explanation is simply that pigment formation is less intense the farther away from the pri- mary centers. The reason of the division of the body surface into these independent areas of pigmentation does not here concern me. It is no doubt the result of physiological causes, and it is rather suggestive that the several patches correspond externally to important nerve centers or groups of nerves. Thus the eye pigment corresponds to the optic nerve, the aural patch to the auditory nerve, so that these two great external sense organs of the head have each their corresponding pigment patch. The neck No. 571] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 393 patch corresponds with the group of cervical nerves, the shoulder patch with the brachial plexus, the side patch with the nerves of the trunk, and the rump patch with the sacral plexus. It may be further suggested that the median crown patch of the head corresponds to the pineal eye, a suggestion that is strengthened by the fact that it is more or less obsolete in mammals, just as the pineal gland is vestigial, whereas. in birds, which are more reptilian in structure, the patch is usually well defined. At all events it is a median unpaired structure, as are the pineal and the interparietal bone. Turning now to a more detailed consideration of these pigment patches in sundry species of animals, we may first examine a series of diagrams (Figs. 1-15) of the j L Wil \ \ i \ | g q 10 t dd Fics. 1-7. DIAGRAMS ILLUSTRATING PIGMENTATION IN THE Domestic Doc. Fics. 8-15. DIAGRAMS ILLUSTRATING PIGMENTATION IN THE DOMESTIC Dog. 394 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII domestic dog, all of which are carefully drawn from photographs or from living animals, and are selected from a great number to show various conditions in the reduction of the pigment patches. In these and the other diagrams the black portions represent pigmented areas, irrespective of the actual colors. For convenience I have called the white stripes demark- ing these chief or primary, patches, ‘‘primary breaks,” since they are the first indications of a decrease in pig- mentation such that two adjoining patches no longer meet. Secondary or further breaks result in a general disintegration of these primary pigment patches, and are apparently more irregular in nature, though often they follow certain fairly well defined lines. The first of the primary breaks generally occur as white patches on the chest or belly, about in the median line. These are not shown in the dagrams, but in most cases should be under- stood as present. In Fig. 1 the pigment areas show a beginning in reduction. The two aural patches have become separated and their failure to spread to the normal limit in the median line has resulted in a white nose stripe. A short transverse white marking indicates a separation of the neck patch at its anterior edge from the ear patch. Elsewhere the various patches are contig- uous; but the extremities of the limbs and tail are pig- mentless, as if pigment had failed to spread to the tips of these members in its reduction. In Fig. 2 the same primary break between the ear patches is present, and in dogs it is one of the first and most frequent to appear. The same shrinkage of pigment from the extremities 1s also seen. The neck patch of the left-hand side, however, has completely dropped out, and its fellow of the right- hand side is reduced posteriorly so that it fails to reach the shoulder patch. Thus a white collar is formed. It is also interesting to see that at its anterior end a distinct constriction is present where the neck patch joins the ear patch of the right side. Fig. 3 shows a somewhat similar condition but the neck patch of the right side as well as No. 571] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 395 the ear patch is missing, while those of the left side are fully developed. In Fig. 4 both neck patches are missing, so that a white collar is formed. In dogs the neck patch is usually the first to drop out altogether, so that a white- collared dog is of very frequent occurrence. In fox hounds this patch is shown unusually well, either wholly or partly separated from neighboring patches. The sep- aration of the ear patches, wholly or partially, so as to produce a white blaze or line in the middle of the forehead is about as frequent. In Figs. 8 and 12 a single neck spot only (as it happens, in one on the right, in the other on the left side) is still present but so slightly developed as to be only a small island of pigment wholly separate from the neighboring patches. The crown spot is so often present in dogs as a little oval island; always on the top of the head about in line with the anterior bases of the ears (Fig. 4) that I am convinced it is a primary patch. It is common in bull dogs and bull terriers, and in other breeds is often seen but is so commonly not indicated at all, that it seems probable it is becoming lost, and its area is filled by the ear patches, since these are often separated by a very narrow median line only, which, as in Fig. 13, may con- tinue posteriorly to separate the two neck patches medially as well. In other cases (Figs. 1, 6) the failure of the white nose stripe to extend farther posteriorly may be due to the persistence of this patch. The demarcation of the side from the rump patches is indicated by the imperfect primary break across the lower part of the back in Fig. 4, while in Fig. 5, a similar primary break farther forward indicates the limits of the shoulder and side patches. In each case the break is incomplete transversely, with a narrow isthmus near the median line. In dogs there is a marked tendency for the ultimate centers of the side and rump patches to be close to the median line, so that the corresponding patches of opposite sides are confluent dorsally. This is especially the case with the rump patches, with the result that it is 396 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII very rare to see the two rump centers separated, but instead, as in Figs. 10 and 14, they appear, when much reduced, as a small median spot at the root of the tail. That they were originally paired, there is no doubt, as there is frequently (as in Fig. 9) a deep median notch indicating the median primary break between the centers, or (as in Figs. 11, 12) one of the lateral centers drops out, leaving its fellow of the opposite side. The ` continued union of the side patches with the shoulder patches is seen in Fig. 7, while in Fig. 9, though the union is still present between these patches of the right side, on the left side the shoulder patch has failed to develop, and the side patch is so reduced that it does not meet its fellow of the right. In Fig. 8 both shoulder patches are present more or less bilaterally equal, and, as frequently, are produced into narrow tongues on to the upper arm. The two side patches in Fig. 8 are also reduced, so as to be wholly separated from each other and from the neigh- boring centers. They are further interesting in being placed nearly median one behind the other instead of nearly opposite. In Fig. 11, on the other hand, they are far sundered, but this, in dogs, is a much less usual con- dition. In Fig. 10 a single median dorsal patch repre- sents the slightly developed side patches, but whether this single patch corresponds to one or other of the two centers, or whether the two are actually fused in the dorsal line, I can not yet say. The shoulder centers, when slightly reduced, are large in dogs, and cover a considerable saddle-shaped area, as indicated in Fig. 5, from near the center of the back for- ward including the fore leg and part of the fore shoulder. When further reduction takes place the pigment is drawn away from the extremities and the saddle separates from the neck patch (Figs. 2, 6) and then from the side patch (Figs. 5, 9), and finally the shoulder patches separate from each other (Fig. 8). One or other of the shoulder patches may drop out entirely (Fig. 10) or be reduced to a very small spot (Fig. 12) at what may be considered No. 571] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 397 the ultimate center of the pigment patch, near the upper part of the body, near or just back of the shoulder. The ear patches seem to be the last to disappear, and these, too, may be variously reduced or only one may be present (Fig. 15). The approximate outlines of the patches when fully developed are indicated by dotted lines in Fig. 15, in which 1 is the crown patch, 2 the ear patch, 3 the neck patch, 4 the shoulder patch, 5 the side patch, and 6 the rump patch. In dogs, there is seldom seen any tendency for these primary patches to divide. What has the appearance of such a tendency is seen, for example, in the coach dog, which is rather evenly flecked with rounded black spots, with often in addition, black ears and more rarely reduced rump patches. Fig. 9 shows such a dog in which both ear patches, one shoulder, both side and both rump patches are sharply indicated, though reduced. In addition there are present on the white body areas between, many small flecks of dark color, evenly distributed, which are clearly not islands separated from the primary patches. Indeed this spotting seems to constitute a wholly different cate- gory of pigment formation, in addition to that of the primary patches, which latter I have called ‘‘centripetal’’ pigmentation. As Professor Castle suggests to me, it is probably homologous with the ‘‘English’’? marking or spotted condition of domesticated rabbits, and possibly the dappling of horses is a similar phenomenon. When these spots and the primary color patches are of the same hue, it is not possible to distinguish the two in visual appearance, unless the latter are reduced areally, when, as is sometimes the case in the coach dog, one or more of the primary patches is seen with the spots, as it were, proliferating from its edge. This second element no doubt enters as a factor in the color pattern when the small spots are of a different color from that of the general body surface, as in case of the cheetah (Cyne- lurus) or the leopard and jaguar. I am inclined to think that the excessive breaking up of 398 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII the primary patches, to be considered under the cow, is not a wholly similar phenomenon. Five diagrams illustrating the domestic cat are shown in Figs. 16 to 20, and are interesting to contrast with ik he I 17 26 Fics. 16-20. DIAGRAMS ILLUSTRATING PIGMENTATION IN THE DOMESTIC CAT. those of the dog, also a carnivorous mammal. The demarcation of the primary patches is usually less sharp than in dogs, but is in general similar. The most.common appearance is where the primary breaks occur in the mid-line below, giving a white throat, chest or belly; or the separation of the aural centers produces a white streak on the nose or extends it up between the ears. The ear patches in Figs. 17, 19, 20, show successive reduc- tion, so that at first the hinder margin of the ears, as in dogs, becomes white, then with further decrease in pig- ment production, the inner bases only are colored. The neck patch has its ultimate center farther back than in dogs so that when much reduced, it is present as a pig- mented spot at the very base of the neck or even at the front of the shoulder (Figs. 16,17). In Fig. 16 the neck patch of the right-hand side is only slightly reduced and is in contact anteriorly with the ear patch, while poste- No. 571] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 399 riorly it does not meet the shoulder patch. The left-hand neck patch, however, is quite separate from the neighbor- ing patches and is reduced to.a small area at the junction of the neck with the shoulder. It is absent in Fig. 17 from the left side and is represented on the right side by a similar small center, placed far back. In Fig. 20 the neck patch or patches show a reduction to a single small Square median patch at the base of the neck, but whether this represents a median fusion of the two lateral centers, or whether one only has persisted and has shifted to the midline, I do not attempt to say, though the former hypothesis seems on the whole more probable. The shoulder patch in house cats is relatively small, and, as indicated by the indentations in Figs. 17, 18, is of the fore side of the upper arm, but the shoulder patch when fully developed seems to cover the rest of the leg and a small scapular area. It is shown much reduced in Fig. 19, on the right-hand side, and is altogether wanting in Fig 20. The conjoined shoulder and side patches in Fig. 18 are shown reduced laterally, so as to form a broad median stripe which I take to mean that the ulti- mate centers are closely approximated dorsally. The neck patch is wholly absent, but both ear patches are present and joined medially. The sacral patches, as commonly, seem fused or at least very close together. There is a small break midway on the tail, which sepa- rates off a pigmented tip, a phenomenon which I shall refer to under ‘‘centrifugal pigmentation.’’ The side patch is long comparatively, and extends forward to cover the deficiencies of the shoulder patch, as in Fig. 17. Here the left side patch has been reduced at its anterior end, and its separateness from the patch of the right side is indicated by the median indentations. It is often want- ing in domesticated cats. o The sacral patches, pigmenting the buttocks and tail, seem to be fused or closely approximated at the root of the tail, as in dogs. I have seen no instance of the crown patch being shown in the cat, though such may occur.. | 400 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVILE The approximate boundaries of the five bilateral patches are indicated in Fig. 20 by dotted lines; 2 is the ear patch, 3 the neck patch, 4 the shoulder patch, 5 the side patch, and 6 the rump patch. Among domesticated rodents the pigment patches have been studied in rats, house mice, and guinea-pigs. In all, the same patches appear except that in rats and mice the median crown patch appears to be lost, though in the guinea-pig it is often present. Diagrams of parti-colored mice are shown in Figs. 21-24, and sufficiently indicate 2/ 2a 23 Fies, 21-24. DIAGRAMS ILLUSTRATING PIGMENTATION IN DOMESTIC VARIETIES OF THE HOUSE MOUSE, the primary pigment areas. The white spot on the fore- head of Fig. 21 indicates a primary break between the two ear patches, and varies widely in different individ- uals, from a few white hairs only to a large blaze The inheritance of such a blaze has been studied by Little (1914). The white mark at the base of the neck in Fig. 21 indicates the beginning of separation of the neck from the shoulder patches and perhaps of the two neck patches from each other, because of its longitudinal extension. The white. band across the neck in Fig. 23, however, indicates probably only the beginning of a separation of the neck from the shoulder patches, which in Fig. 24 has No. 571] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 401 wholly sundered these two areas, so that a white-collared mouse results. The condition shown in Fig. 22 is similar, except that the separation has taken place on the right side only, between the neck and the shoulder patches of but one half of the body. A break between the two neck patches of opposite sides is further indicated in this figure by the deep median reentrant back of the ears. In all four diagrams the areal restriction of the shoulder patches is shown, but in varying degrees. In Fig. 21, the pigment has not spread to the feet, leaving these white, and so in the other figures, but to a greater degree. A median linear break between the shoulders indicates the restriction of the patches of opposite sides at this point, which in Fig. 22 is more clearly perceptible. The posterior limits of the shoulder patch are further shown in this diagram, by the beginnings of a break between the shoulder and the side patches. In Fig. 24 this break is no longer interrupted, but clearly separates the two areas. Further, the side patch has dropped out on the left. In Fig. 23 an imperfect separation of patches on the posterior part of the body has taken place. On the right-hand side the shoulder patch, which in mice is of considerable extent, has broadly separated from the side patch, while on the left-hand side a long transverse break has taken place between the side and the rump patches, with two island-like white spots between, the anterior of which probably marks the transverse line of stress between shoulder and side patches, the posterior the median line of breaking between the two side patches. A slight indentation in the pigmented area far back on the right side of Fig. 22 points to the beginning of restric- tion between side patch and rump patch. The separation of these patches by a transverse mid-dorsal break is shown in Fig. 21, and their complete separation on the left side appears in Fig. 23 (the transverse white mark), while in Fig. 24, owing to the failure of the left-hand pleural patch to develop, the two rump patches, both par- tially separate from each other, are wholly disconnected 402 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII from the former except by a narrow isthmus on the right side. The long tail is usually without pigment, or mainly so Where areal restriction is present, and it is seldom that pigment extends far on to the base of this member when the restrictive tendency appears. In the domesticated varieties of rats, the same patches may be distinguished. There is, however, an interesting variety known as the ‘‘hooded” rat, in which the ear and neck patches appear to be normal, but a narrow median dorsal area is pig- mented for a varying length, sometimes quite to the root of the tail. A separate factor seems here to be involved, producing what may be called a ‘‘centrifugal’’ type of pigmentation, which in many forms of mammals causes a black spine stripe (Sorex wardi, Tupaia tana, certain forms of Apodemus, Equus caballus), and others. Among guinea-pigs the typical primary patches are beautifully shown and may be seen in sundry figures published in papers by Professor Castle on heredity in this animal. The guinea-pig is one of the few mammals yet known in which the median crown patch is visibly present, a character which I take to be primitive. In guinea-pigs the breaking up of the ticked color pattern has progressed under long domestication to an extraordinary degree, so that not only are black, tawny or grizzled animals produced in various shades, but even in the same individual, the different primary pigment areas may be of different colors. This fact is of much significance, for it indicates not only the mutual independ- ence of the contiguous color areas, but further points to the manner in which a variegated color pattern may have been acquired. Among mammals the color pattern is in general, not greatly developed in comparison with birds, yet in many cases where some modification has taken place, it is evident that this differentiation is confined to the limits of one or two of the primary pigment patches. Thus in the South American Tayra (T. barbara), the head and neck are a grizzled gray, and the breaks occur- ring in pied individuals show that the grizzled condition No. 571] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 403 FIGs, 25-32. DIAGRAMS ILLUSTRATING PIGMENTATION IN HORSES, 404 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII is confined to the aural and nuchal patches only, for else- where the animal is black. In this case, too, the black condition is probably derived, for youngish animals are uniformly grizzled, and sometimes, apparently, this is the adult condition as well. Among domesticated ungulates the same primary patches are to be distinguished in cases where partial albinism renders their bounds apparent, with the excep- tion that in horses, cows and deer I have seen no clear indication of the median crown patch which in mammals is probably obsolescent. In both horses and cows the patches show interesting and peculiar modifications. A series of diagrams (Figs. 25 to 32) show these patches in ‘‘calico’’ horses, though not so fully as could be wished. The first indications of areal restriction of pigment in horses appear in the shape of a white ‘‘star’’ or round spot in the center of the fore- head. This is often accompanied by white at the base of the hoofs, or sometimes the entire foot is white producing the so-called ‘‘white stockings.’’ But there is no neces- sary correlation between these white areas, such as Brewer (1882) tried to show. The white on the forehead may vary from a few white hairs to a broad blaze cover- ing the entire front of the head between the eyes to the muzzle. Sometimes the restriction of pigment is such as to produce in addition to the white star on the forehead, a white spot over each eye, and sometimes these three spots are joined by a narrow unpigmented area. This indicates that pigment production is weak at a spot directly over the eye in comparison with neighboring parts, and this no doubt accounts for the fact that in black-and-tan or other dogs these are the pale spots over the eyes where black pigment is not produced. A white spot over the eye is also characteristic of many rodents. Next after the restriction of the ear patches and the drawing away of pigment from the feet, the most common No. 571] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 405 white marking seems to be a primary break, as in Fig. 25, from the shoulder back of the foreleg, which delimits the posterior border of the shoulder patch. In the horse the shoulder patch is large, and differs from that of any mammal I have yet studied, in its great extent forward along the dorsal side of the neck nearly to the head. In Fig. 26 a small break at the back of the neck indicates the beginning of separation between the ear and the neck patches dorsally, and a long tongue of white running up- ward from the forearm indicates the anterior limit of the shoulder patch. This limit is marked still nearer the dorsal line in Fig. 27 by a white spot on the side of the ` neck near its base. In Fig. 28 the shoulder patch has entirely dropped out and the white space outlines very nearly its extent. The ultimate center is perhaps shown by the small shoulder spot in Fig. 31. The area covered by the ear patches extends well on to the upper part of the neck, and in Fig. 29 is shown at its greatest spread, or, as in Fig. 28, cut off by anarrow white collar from the neck patch. The neck patch is remarkable from the fact that in its areal reduction it becomes re- stricted first dorsally, and the ultimate center of each side is nearly ventral on the throat, so that, as generally seen, the two centers form a single median patch on the front or ventral part of the throat. In Fig. 26 the neck patch is seen to pigment the anterior side of the forearm and is partly separated from the shoulder patch by a long tongue of white. It seems to extend up diagonally to reach the mid-line of the neck for a short distance only, as indicated in Fig. 28, where its bounds are only slightly contracted. In Fig. 29 it is so far lessened as to be absent from the forearm, though still in contact at the throat with the ear patch where, however, a deep indentation locates the dividing line between the two patches. In Fig. 31 a median ventral division of the conjoined neck patches is seen indicated at the upper part of the area, which in this case no longer reaches the ear patches. Still further reduction of both ear patches and neck 406 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII patches is seen in Fig. 32, but, as commonly, the neck patches seem fused in the midventral line. This shifting of the neck centers ventrally is a rather remarkable phenomenon which may have some relation to the manner in which the head is held erect. For this reason it might be expected also in antelopes, and is perhaps evidenced in such a species as the oryx, in which there is a black median line on the throat as though strongest pigment production centered there rather than on the gray sides of the neck. The median reduction of the shoulder patches in horses is sometimes indicated by a white mane. The rump patches in the horse appear to be much as in other mammals, restricted to the tail and posterior part’ of the buttocks and the entire foot. In Fig. 30 the patch is shown at nearly its full development, except that it has failed to extend to the entire hind foot. In Fig. 27 it has drawn away still farther but remains in contact with the side patch at one place. In Fig. 29 it is further restricted to the tail and posterior border of the haunches, while in Fig. 32 it covers only the root of the tail and that member. The side patch is the largest of all and extends from the shoulder to the fore part of the haunches and on to the fore part of the hind leg nearly to the foot, as seen in Figs. 27 and 28, where it is still in contact with the rump patch, or in Fig. 29 where it has become separated. In its fur- ther reduction this patch may appear as a small spot back of the ribs or, as often, a curious division takes place, separating the patch into a dorsal area and a lateral one. Occasionally this secondary break appears in a horse which has most of its patches otherwise well developed. In Fig. 31, the pigmented area of the tail, buttocks and lumbar region consists of the conjoined rump patch and a dorsal portion of the side patch, while the ventral part of the side patch is present as the oval spot at the groin. In Fig. 30 the latter spot only persists, but in Fig. 32 the dorsal portion of the side patch alone is present as a stripe along the entire back, except where it breaks away posteriorly from the small rump patch. No. 571] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 407 This peculiarity of the side patch in horses is somewhat paralleled in cows by a tendency to secondary breaking up, though in a different way, as detailed below. It is significant in this connection that in horses and donkeys there is usually a black stripe along the spine from shoulder to tail which may indicate that ‘‘centrifugal pig- mentation’’ is also present (see beyond). The dotted lines in Fig. 32 indicate the approximate boundaries of the several primary patches. The crown patch seems to be wanting in horses; 2 is the ear patch, 3 the neck patch, 4, 5 and 6 the shoulder, side, and rump patches, respectively. : . Of domestic ruminants I have studied the pigmentation in the cow and show in Figs. 33 to 42 a few of the many Fics. 33-36. DIAGRAMS ILLUSTRATING PIGMENTATION IN DOMESTIC Cows, SIDE VIEW. variations in partial pigmentation. These are all drawn from photographs or from the animals themselves, and are of cows in which, so far as I know, there has been no attempt at breeding for pattern. Two types of spotting may be distinguished in cows: first, that in which the pig- mented areas are sharply outlined and solid or at least 408 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII practically so; second, that in which there is a greater or less tendency for the primary patches to be much broken | up into small islands (as in Fig. 36) by secondary breaks, though the main areas are still distinguishable. I take this second or fragmental type to be a different phenom- enon from the diffuse or dappled condition seen in the coach dog or the dappled-gray horse. In the cow, the ear patches as usual pigment each its proper side of the head to a short distance behind the Hics. 37-42. DIAGRAMS ILLUSTRATING PIGMENTATION IN Domestic Cows, AS SEEN SPREAD OUT AND FROM ABOVE. ears. The point of separation between ear patches and neck patches is indicated by a small break back of the skull in Fig. 38, while the posterior extent is shown by the two ear patches in Fig. 42. These patches usually No. 571] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 409 draw apart first across the forehead making here a tri- angular white mark, and on the muzzle, as in Fig. 34. Further restriction broadens these white marks and joins them by a narrow isthmus as in Fig. 35. In Fig. 40, the two patches are still conjoined across the vertex, but are much reduced, that of the right side more than that of the left. In Fig. 42 they have failed to join medially, though fairly well developed longitudinally. Still greater re- duction, as in Fig. 37, confines them to the ears, the bases of which appear to be the ultimate centers. The neck patch in the cow is more extended posteriorly than in the horse, and its center is strictly lateral rather than nearly ventral. It is shown in Fig. 34 somewhat contracted from the mid-line of the throat, but extends squarely back against the foreshoulder at the base of the neck, and is fused near its ventral corner with the small shoulder patch, itself much reduced. As in other mammals it appears to extend in its complete development, to the front edge of the upper foreleg. The animal in Fig. 41 shows a bilaterality in its pigmentation that is rather unusual. What appear to be the reduced neck patches are seen far back at the border of the fore- shoulder. In Fig. 40 the left-hand neck patch has dropped out, but that of the right side is still present, though small, and in Fig. 42 it is reduced to a small spot only. The shoulder patch in cows is remarkably narrow, and compressed between the neck patch and the body patch, whence it extends as usual on to the foreleg. In Fig. 33 a primary break back of the foreshoulder marks the nearly vertical posterior outline of the shoulder patch. In Fig. 34 the separation of this area from the neck patch is all but complete and the patch itself some- what reduced. Its narrow vertical outline is thus indi- cated, as well as in Fig. 39, in which there is a narrow tongue-like extension down on to the center of the foreleg. In its further reduction it appears as a small center at the base of the scapula, as in Fig. 35, or in Fig. 40, in 410 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII which both shoulder patches are present, though small. In Figs. 41 and 42 the shoulder areas are wanting. A very common mark in cows is a white belt just back of the foreleg. This is due to the development of a primary break between shoulder patches and side patches, a con- dition which is nearly realized in Figs. 33 and 38. It is probable that this marking has been more or less fixed through selection in breeding, and this has been the more readily accomplished, since this break occurs in a place which is one of the first in cows to cease pigment production. The side patch is large and covers the entire lateral region of the body from the scapula to the hips, and on to the front edge of the hind limb. When only slightly reduced, it appears as a blanket-shaped area across the back as in Fig. 38, where it has not wholly broken away from the shoulder and rump patches, or as in Fig. 33, where it has become nearly separated. In its further reduction this dorsal blanket shows a peculiar manner of breaking up into more or less transverse stripes directed slightly backward. The beginnings of these secondary breaks appear in Fig. 39 in which are seen on each side posteriorly two deep indentations at the edge of the patch, whose points if extended would meet the white pigmentless islands already present within the patch. In Fig. 34 a similar series of indentations points to the trisection of the side patch which is realized in Fig. 35. Here is a characteristic which if developed might even- tually result in the actual production of white stripes on the body, such as are found, for example, in certain ante- lopes as the bongo and the kudu. The tendency of the side patch to divide into three, as in these diagrams, is rather marked in cows, and even with further reduction the three centers persist fairly well. The first of these secondary centers is just back of the shoulder patch, the second about over the last ribs, and the third over the lumbar region. In Fig. 40 the first two are present on the left side, with a small spot between, which has become No. 571] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 41] separated from one or the other of them, while the third or lumbar spot has dropped out. On the right side, the first and second divisions are still fused dorsally, but the lumbar division is distinct. The same three divisions are seen in Fig. 35, better developed, whereas in Fig. 42, the two lumbars are present, one on each side, and con- siderably in advance of them, what seem to be the rem- nants of the first division of the side area, the left one of which has further broken up. The rump patches show no especial peculiarities, but cover the posterior part of the buttocks and hind legs, and the entire feet and tail. Though frequently the two patches of opposite sides are conjoined medially, they are often, under considerable reduction, well separated. The beginning of such a separation appears in Fig. 38, where there is a deep median tongue of white anteriorly, mark- ing the line of union. In Fig. 41 the reduction has pro- gressed still farther so that the two patches are quite sun- dered medially and do not extend to the tail. In Fig. 40 the patch of the left side has become inactive, and that of the right side is small. A curious condition not infrequently seen is shown in Fig. 37, in which all the patches are present, but those of the right side are separated from those of the left by a median dorsal white line, showing the distinct bilaterality of these pigment areas. In the figure, the ear patches are so restricted as not to reach the neck patches of their respective sides, the shoulder patches do not extend far on the forelegs, the side patches are reduced ven- trally, and the rump patches, though in contact with the side patches, do not pigment the tail or extremities of the legs. A further reduction of pigment areas results in Fig. 41, in which the paired centers of neck, side and rump patches still appear. The diffuse condition of pigmentation is illustrated in Fig. 36, which is a photograph, inked in. The ear patch is seen much reduced, but pigmenting the ear. The neck patch is of most irregular shape, with several subsidiary 412 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII spots separated from its lower border. A clear line separates the neck patch from the shoulder patch, which is also of most irregular boundary. The side patch, at its fore part, is broken into a series of small islands which tend to arrange themselves in lines following the direction of the ribs. The main part of the patch shows a decided tendency to break into the usual three or per- haps four portions. It is common for cows to have patches with very irregular boundaries and tongues of pigment, which may break off into isolated spots in a most bewildering fashion, but even in such cases it is possible to distinguish the main patches of which these form part. White patches occur in other domesticated ungulates as the pig, the llama, the alpaca, the camel, the yak, the reindeer, and the goat. In the water-buffalo, occasional animals seen in Egypt show a beginning of pigment re- duction through the presence of white in the forehead or on the tail. I have had no opportunity to study the mark- ings of these species. (To be concluded) . INTERNAL RELATIONS OF TERRESTRIAL ASSOCIATIONS ARTHUR G. VESTAL UNIVERSITY OF COLORADO CONTENTS I. Introduction, II. Internal activities of the association, as determined by the con- stitution of the individual organism. e organism. - Ecolo B. Farnar of the plant in relation to environment. Constitution of the ppap: s relation to environment. Internal annie of the III, Relative spe of different segues with the association——domi- nan A. enon of dominance e animals. iteria of dominance among animals. C. Spe cialized and unspecialized animals. IV. a ee in the association Ai 4 in 5 ace. ribution me. Va Tatevdependencs of atrak trial plant and animal communities. Aces rela spies of At ined “agen and animals. eographie range: the pro £ Distsbation apre the province: distribution of plants and an ane communitie B. Local relations Re plant and kaia assemblages (relations n the esl an Similarity of ecological type of plants and animals. 2. Relative hes smog ce of plant and animal a ssemblages. 3. Correspondence in distribution within the sane ciation. 4. Un porns ic of species composition of plant and animal VE oe pate pres aor VII. References I. INTRODUCTION THE material here presented is based on the writer’s studies, during the past five years, of terrestrial associa- tions of plants and animals, mainly in different parts of the prairie region. The particular area chiefly used for illustration in this paper is the sand prairie of the Illinois River valley, plants and animals of which have been studied by Hart and Gleason (1907) and by the writer (1913b). A later study has been made of the vegetation of inland sand areas of Illinois (Gleason, 1910) ; the Lake Michigan beach area in northeastern Illinois has been studied by Gates (1912) ; beach areas in Illinois and Indi- ana by the writer (1914a). The chief representation of 413 414 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVI the sand prairie is the bunch-grass association, well- developed in parts of northwestern, central and north- eastern Illinois, and in northwestern Indiana, in each of which areas, as well as in the sandhills of Nebraska and of eastern Colorado, the writer has studied. Discus- sions of physical, vegetational and animal aspects of the associations of the central Illinois sand prairie, together with an annotated list of the animal species, with data on food, habitat-relations, life-history, ete., are embodied in the writer’s paper (1913b), to which constant reference is made. Frequent citations to a more detailed study of local distribution of grasshoppers, in a Michigan area (Vestal, 1913a), and to the many associational studies of Shelford, are to be found. The data wal have accumulated relate nearly equally to the botanical and zoological aspects of associational study, but since the subject of plant ecology is at present more advanced than that of animal ecology, it has been possible to treat the vegetational side of the problem very briefly, so that more of the discussion relates to animals and animal assemblages. The writings most frequently cited are indicated by italic capitals, the full titles appearing in the list of spe- cial references at the end of the paper. The writer wishes to thank Dr. Charles C. Adams, Dr. Max M. Ellis and Dr. H. A. Gleason for suggestions and criticism. II. INTERNAL ACTIVITIES OF THE ASSOCIATION, AS DETER- INED BY THE CONSTITUTION OF THE INDIVIDUAL ORGANISM The internal activities of the association may be said to be the sum-total of the activities of all the plants and all the animals which make up the association. Such a sum-total of activities may well be thought of as an intri- cate and complicated mass of dependencies. It will simplify the treatment of the entire system of relations if the chief dependencies of the individual organism are first discussed. A knowledge of the ecology of the asso- No. 571] TERRESTRIAL ASSOCIATIONS 415 ciation is built up largely from a knowledge of the ecol- ogy of all the organisms which compose it. A. Ecouocican CONSTITUTION OF THE ORGANISM The constitution of the organism is the sum-total of those of its characters which enter into relation with environment. These are commonly classified as structural and physiological. For the purposes of this discussion it would seem preferable to subdivide physiological char- acters, restricting the term physiological to denote those characters concerned with ordinary metabolic processes of the organism, and excluding those having to do with life-history and rates of reproduction (these may be dis- tinguished as biographical and numerical') and also, when dealing with animals, those related to behavior (psycho- logical characters). The constitution of the organism in relation to environment will be discussed in terms of these classes of characters. B. CONSTITUTION oF THE PLANT IN RELATION TO ENVIRONMENT -~ The environmental influences in the association are of three kinds: (1) physical, (2) plant, (3) animal. Each plant and each animal must obtain from each of these three constituents of its environment certain necessaries; it has certain structural and physiological characters which enable it to obtain these necessaries, and to with- stand adverse environmental influences. The environmental relations of plants are very differ- ent from those of animals. A tabular comparison of these relations has been made by Shelford (4: 593). As therein pointed out, structural characters are of greatest importance in the adjustment of the plant to the environ- ment, and plants in a given habitat are likely to have a common structure or growth-form, indicating common or ecologically equivalent physiological conditions within. Different plants (and different animals), within a com- 1 Based partly on Forbes’, classification of adaptation to food require- ments (1909: 292). 416 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVII mon habitat, are similar in ecological constitution (eco- logically equivalent) in so far as their presence is deter- mined by the same environmental conditions. It should be pointed out that there are local environmental differ- ences within the area of the association which allow the presence of differently constituted organisms, and that the entire range of environmental conditions within the habitat is usually much wider than that of the environ- mental complex selected by a particular organism. The environmental complex of the organism is not the same as the sum-total of environmental conditions within the association. Each organism differs in greater or less degree from others in ecological constitution, and thus selects a different environmental complex. The physical factors of the environment are of great- est importance in the life of the plant. Plants influence one another directly to only a slight extent. There is usually very little of the social relationship among eco- logically similar plants which will compare with such relationships as seen in animals. Competition among plants is mainly a struggle to determine which plants are to be most favored by physical conditions, and it is probably most severe for the physical factor present in minimal quantity. In desert associations plant competi- tion is almost exclusively for water, and extensive root systems are developed. In grassland it is very largely for above-ground space; in forests it is principally for light. The influence of the animal-environment is prob- ably of greater importance than has commonly been realized by plant ecologists; the study of economic ento- mology and of the effects of grazing upon grasslands is helping to bring about a realization of the importance of animal influence upon plant life. The structures of plants show frequent and great modification in response to the physical conditions of the environment. These modifications are most frequent and important with respect to the factor present in minimal quantity. Characters which may be associated with No. 571] TERRESTRIAL ASSOCIATIONS 417 direct plant influence are infrequent. Certain plants which become more abundant as a result of close grazing are equipped with spines, or have acrid or pungent juices; and many other characters may be correlated with animal influence. The structural modifications are most evident in the adjustment of the plant to external conditions, though these are accompanied by physiolog- ical pharaetens which are also in harmony with the en- vironment. C. CONSTITUTION oF THE ANIMAL IN RELATION TO ENVIRONMENT The animal, like the plant, selects an environmental complex which is of three kinds: (1) physical, (2) plant, (3) animal. Different animals show extreme variation as to the degree in which the different parts of the en- vironment are important to their existence. Endopara- sites, for example, are most directly concerned with the animal part of their environmental complex. The existence of any animal is dependent upon a num- ber of physical factors, all of which must be present in proper degree or quantity. Minimal and maximal quan- tities of any one of several factors mark the limits of existence of any animal (A: 598—law of toleration of physical factors). It is not necessary to consider these factors in detail. The animal reacts to physical environ- ment most evidently by its behavior: psychological char- acters restrict activities more narrowly than do those of other types. They are accompanied by structural and physiological characters; hibernation, storage of food, etc., are biographical characters correlated with seasonal changes in physical environment. Animals which are subjected to very severe physical conditions may produce a larger number of offspring than those to which physical conditions are favorable. This is an example of corre- lation of a numerical character with the physical environ- ment, The plant environment reacts upon and modifies phys- ical and animal environments, and has also direct influ. 418- THE AMERICAN NATURALIST. [Vou. XLVII ence upon the animal. In addition to its effect in the con- trol of temperature, light and other physical factors, the vegetation ‘constitutes the basic food-supply for the ani- mal community, and also provides shelter and materials for abode (A: 601). Cases of direct association between particular plants and particular animals are numerous, but the majority of animals have no direct relation to particular kinds of plants. Behavior characters are in general of greater importance in the relation of the animal to the plant environment, though such relations are not confined to psychological characters. There are two sets of relations between the animal and its animal environment. These are: (1) social, and (2) antagonistic. Social relations (inter-psychology and inter-physiology of Shelford, A: 608, b) include those between individuals of the same species, and between animals of the same or similar mores? (ecologically equiv- alent animals), in so far as these relations are not antagonistic. Breeding and family relations are the principal activities which come under this head. Be- havior characters are of greatest importance, as compared with structural and other characters. The antagonistic relations constitute the intermores-psychology and phys- iology of Shelford (A: 608, c). They are the antagonistic relations between animals not ecologically equivalent, and they are also antagonistic relations within a species and between ecologically similar forms. These relations are probably not greatly concerned with reproduction, but center about the feeding activities of the animal. The existence of the individual animal, in its relation to other organisms, is dependent upon three conditions: (1) it must obtain suitable and sufficient food, (2) it must be free from destructive competition of animals of similar requirements, (3) it must be able to escape or to with- stand attacks of other animals (or, sometimes, of para- sitic fungi or bacteria). The various characters of the 2 Mores (Latin for customs, habits) has been used by Shelford (1911a: 30) to supply the need for a term including all physiological and behavior characters of the animal. s No. 571] TERRESTRIAL ASSOCIATIONS 419 animals are correlated with all three of these conditions. The characters are both ‘‘adaptive’’ (fixed by heredity), and regulatory (not fixed). Following is a synopsis of correlations between the various types of characters and the three conditions of existence, in the relation of the animal to its antagonistic animal environment. (I) Characters Which Enable the Animal to Obtain Food 1. Structural Characters—Animals of selective food- habits often have specialized structures, as in the case of the long tongue of woodpeckers. Animals of non-selec- tive food-habits have mouthparts that are not so highly specialized; thus grasshoppers and cutworms have heavy mandibles for cutting vegetation; tiger-beetles and Chrysopa larve have sharp piercing mandibles. The whole structure of the predaceous animal, its ‘‘action system,’’ is sometimes suggestive of the manner of pur- suit or holding of its prey. ` 2. Physiological Characters—The physiology of ani- mals of different food-habits differs materially. Physio- logical characters are not apparent, generally speaking, and are secondary to psychological characters. The range of food assimilable by the animal is usually much wider than that selected by it, as is seen when animals of selective habits take new kinds of food when the usual food is exhausted, often thriving seemingly as well as before. 3. Psychological Characters.—Selection of food is determined chiefly by behavior characters of the animal. These may be so widely variable that the animal will be virtually omnivorous, as in the case of crickets, or so narrowly restricted that it eats only a single species of plant or animal, as the leaf-beetle Blepharida, a sand- prairie insect eating leaves of the three-lobed sumac, and the pentatomid bug, Perillus, which feeds on Blepharida (cf. E: 49, 30). Selection is only one of the many psycho- logical characters relating to food. The behavior cnar- acters manifested in obtaining food are of great variety. 420 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vor. XLVII With these are accompanying structural and physiolog- ical characters, which, however, play a subordinate part. 4. Biographical Characters—These may consist in timing the life-history of the animal with that of the food-species (plant or animal) in such a way that the period of greatest activity of the former coincides with the period of greatest growth or abundance of the latter. This feature may be incidental to seasonal change of physical environment. Whatever its cause, it is very general in an established association, so general that it is seldom recognized. It is of advantage to both animal and food species. 5. Numerical Characters—The rate of reproduction must be so adjusted to its food-supply (plant or animal) ‘that only the unessential surplus of this food shall be appropriated, leaving the essential maximum product undiminished’? (Forbes, 1909: 293). Species of re- stricted food-habits must remain less numerous in indi- viduals than general feeders, as the available food-supply | is very much less. (II) Characters Which Remove the Animal from the ompetition of Other Forms 1. Structural Characters.—Structures which permit animals to live in varied habitats, to take varied foods, or to time their activities differently, remove each group of animals from competition of all the others, resulting in advantage to all. To that extent the fossorial forelegs of the mole, the long proboscis of the butterfly, and modi- fications of the eyes of nocturnal animals, are characters which do away with competition. The structural char- acters are, however, accompaniments of modifications of behavior, and are secondary to the latter. 2. Physiological Characters.—Ability to digest food- materials unavailable to other animals is an advantage- ous physiological character. Thus the leaf-beetle Chry- sochus auratus, which lives on doghane (Apocynum), and the ‘‘skin-beetle’’ Tros, which eats animal tissues in an advanced stage of decomposition, have few com- No. 571] TERRESTRIAL ASSOCIATIONS 421 petitors for food. Physiological, as well as structural, characters, are accompaniments to modifications of habit. 3. Psychological Characters—Apparent preference for certain activities, certain habitats, or certain foods, together with peculiar behavior complexes, seem to be of greater importance in removing animals from competi- tion than structural and physiological characters. Highly regulatory habits permit certain animals to ad- just themselves to changing conditions of competition. 4. Biographical Characters.—Professor Forbes (1909: 295-298) discusses the alternative timing of the active period among close competitors for food. (It so happens that the animals mentioned, having almost identical habits, compete with each other in many ways, besides with respect to food.) In the sand prairie it has been found that different species of certain genera, having otherwise the same habits, differ greatly in life-history. Evidence of this biographical adjustment is more or less complete for two species of Arphia (E: 21), two or three species of Hippiscus (E : 21), two species of the milkweed beetle, Tetraopes (E: 47), and three species of Procta- canthus, robber-flies (E: 55). In these genera the term of activity of one species is abruptly followed by that of another, the successive periods usually covering most of the summer season. 5. Numerical Characters—When a certain limited food, place of abode, or other desideratum is used by two or more kinds of animals at one time, a numerical adjust- ment is likely to be found among these competing species. The rate of multiplication of each species must be suffi- cient to keep up its numbers, to allow it to hold place with competing species. (Too high rates of multiplica- tion, on the other hand, are disadvantageous because of other influences.) (III) Protective, Defensive and Concealing Characters 1. Structural Characters—Animals have various de- fensive, protective and concealing structures. Stings, beaks, mandibles, teeth, claws, hairs, spines, resemblance 422 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII to surroundings in color or form—all are of advantage to animals which possess them. Certain of the interstitial or blowsand animals resemble in color the sand on which they rest (Cicindela lepida, Stachyocnemis, Psinidia, Spharagemon; cf. E). 2. Physiological Characters—Malodorous and ill- tasting animals are to a considerable degree exempt from attack. This is essentially a physiological modification, though a structural basis in the form of glands may be ‘present. In the sand prairie Chrysopa (lace-winged fly), a number of Hemiptera, ladybird beetles, soldier bugs (Chauliognathus), blister-beetles (Epicauta), and others, are ill-tasting (perhaps not to some animals). The skunk’s lack of caution is well known. 3. Psychological Characters.—Self-preservation in animals depends more upon their activities and behavior than upon special structures. The ordinary methods of resisting or evading attacks of enemies are generally known and need not be discussed. Many specialized in- stincts have arisen, such as feigning death, or dropping to the ground when disturbed, as seen in many herbi- colous beetles. 4. Biographical Characters. —It is to the advantage of animal species preyed upon by others if their period of greatest abundance is timed with the period of greatest activity of the animals which feed upon them. 5. Numerical Characters.—Animals, as well as plants, must produce a normal excess in numbers which will pro- vide food for other animals and still leave a sufficient number of individuals to continue the species. It will be noted that the various kinds of characters usually accompany.one another, all being parts of a single modification. This modification may have rela- tion to one or to several of the environmental influences (physical, plant or animal) or to more than one kind of antagonistic relation between the animal and others. The modification is not necessarily advantageous to the animal with respect to all or to any features of the No. 571] TERRESTRIAL ASSOCIATIONS 423 environment, though a large number of characters do result in advantage. Characters advantageous in one relation may be disadvantageous or indifferent in an- other relation. The origin of the characters is not at present a subject which can be treated in a study of inter- relations of organisms (cf. Shelford, 1912b: 342). Be- havior characters appear to be of greatest importance to the animal in determining its relations with other organ- isms of the association, though usually these are accom- panied by physiological or structural characters. The animal is not adapted to a particular status in the asso- ciation; its ecological constitution determines what place it shall be able to find among the other animals of its sur- roundings. The relations among the various animals, when a state of equilibrium has been reached, are the result of mutual accommodation | on the part of all the animals involved. D. [INTERNAL Activities oF THE ASSOCIATION It has been indicated that the complex of activities within the association is the synthesis of all the activities of the individual organisms. Each plant and each ani- mal is subjected to physical, plant and animal influences. From the extreme complexity of the entire system of relations within the association, it is hardly possible to consider more than one or several of these at one time.* It is possible, however, to see that each species finds a status within the association, according to its particular combination of internal and external relations. It con- tinues in fairly constant numbers from year to year. A change in these numbers, if at all great, may cause a dis- turbance in the association, which is quickly regulated by the activities of conflicting organisms (Forbes, 1880). The entire association of plants and animals, by very ' 8 Very helpful diagrams are given by Shelford (C: 167, 168) which il- lustrate the food relations of land (prairie) animals. There are also dia- grams showing food relations of aquatic animals (C: 70, 71). Food rela- tions of animals of plains and mountain streams are discussed by Ellis (1914: 122-127; diagram on p. 125). References to studies dealing with interrelations Sy organisms may be found in the recent handbook of Adams (1913: 123 et seq.). 424 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou, XLVII reason of the conflicting interests, the varying conditions necessary for existence, and the varying methods of re- sponse to these conditions, forms a self-contained and self-regulating system of activities. III. RELATIVE INFLUENCE OF DIFFERENT ORGANISMS WITHIN ASSOCIA TION—DOMINANCE The plant ecologist determines which plants in an asso- ciation are of greatest importance (dominant) by ob- serving which species tend to increase at the expense of others, which are most abundant, most frequent, largest, ete. Competition among plants in a grassland associa- tion is mainly for space, and the dominant species are usually determined with considerable accuracy after some study. With the animals the consideration of dominance involves greater complexity. The important relations between conflicting animal species are those in which they obtain food, are removed from competition, or escape enemies. These relations are in each case most - directly concerned with food. The plant-eaters of the association thus form a dominant group within the asso- ciation, since predaceous and parasitic animals, and scavengers in large part, depend upon them for existence. Individual species within the various food-groups, how- ever, present such striking differences in importance, that we can not speak of all plant-eaters as dominant forms, or that all animals of other food-habits are un- important. It is merely probable that the phytophagous group will contain a larger proportion of dominant spe- cies. This appears to be the condition in the bunch-grass association. A. Factors or DOMINANCE AMONG ANIMALS The success of an animal species within an association is due to the resultant effect of a large number of factors. Among these may be mentioned number of individuals, size, activity, voracity, concentration of food, rapidity of growth, rapidity of reproduction, and wideness of dis- tribution in space and in time. Dominance signifies more No. 571] TERRESTRIAL ASSOCIATIONS 425 than mere ability of a species to thrive in its surround- ings: the species of greatest influence are those on which the greatest number of other animals depend; thus domi- nant species are successful, but successful species are not always dominant. Species which are relatively free from competition or which have comparatively few enemies may be successful, but are not dominant, and are usually not numerous. Species which are successful and at the same time extremely abundant, usually form the food of a large number of other animals, as it appears to be the rule that no considerable source of food within the asso- ciation is left unused. Dominance in a species, then, would seem to include the dependence of other animals upon it, plus the ability to thrive in spite of the drain upon its numbers. B. Crrrerta or DOMINANCE AMONG ANIMALS The factors mentioned as contributing to the success of a species, and the numbers of animals dependent upon the species, are all indications of the degree of its domi- nance. It appears that another criterion is available, which perhaps expresses the summation of many factors which contribute toward dominance. This is the degree of specialization exhibited by the species in its adjust- ment to a particular place in the association. Dominant animals appear to be those of moderately specialized habits rather than those of highly specialized, or rela- tively unspecialized, habits. C. ĶPECIALIZED AND UNSPECIALIZED ANIMALS Each species may be referred to a position in the scale of specialization in habit. The degree of specialization of the species is well seen in the food-habits, though all the habits are to be considered. The most abundant food in the sand prairie is plant material, bunch-grasses. The majority of the plant-feeders are adapted to eat herbage of nearly any kind: they are not restricted to particular species or particular parts of plants. They are non- selective feeders. Grasshoppers, cutworms and certain ~ 426 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVII leaf-beetles are thus moderately specialized plant-eaters. There are also non-selective predaceous animals, as tiger- beetles and lycosid spiders, which eat any kind of small animal. These are also moderately specialized. The moderately specialized animals carry on the gross metab- olism of the association; they constitute the dominant group, and include the dominant species. Selective feeders belong with the highly specialized animals. In the bunch-grass association Languria bi- color, an erotylid beetle, bores in the stems of the com- posite Cacalia (Indian plantain), while Lygeus bicrucis (hemipterous) feeds on the same plant; Perillus circwm- cinctus eats Blepharida rhois. Others of the associa- tion eat selectively. The majority of parasites are greatly restricted in their selection of hosts. Such ani- mals are particularly dependent upon special kinds of food, which in many cases are not available to general feeders. Highly specialized forms are thus enabled to avail themselves of opportunities denied to animals of generalized type; but while they avoid competition by the adoption of special kinds of food, or by special habit of some other kind, they lack the versatility of the less specialized animals, being unable to adjust themselves to changed conditions. They may, therefore, become abundant at times; but as they depend wholly upon one variable condition (perhaps the presence of a particular plant species, which may be quite infrequent) they never can become dominant species. Absolute numbers of the insects which live upon Cacalia, for example, are insig- nificant in comparison with such animals as the grass- hoppers. On the other hand, animals of relatively non-specialized habits would also be ineffective in the association, for whatever field of activity they were to enter, they usu- ally would find already occupied by some animal better constituted for that activity. Such non-specialized forms would assume particular importance only when some animal on which they might feed should become unusually No. 571] TERRESTRIAL ASSOCIATIONS 427 abundant. Few animals are really non-specialized in habits; many moderately specialized species, however, may on occasion turn from their ordinary activities, per- haps to appropriate a particularly abundant kind of food. Many ants are thus habituated to certain ordinary kinds of food, but are able to eat organic food of almost any sort, and do vary their food with circumstance. When, as frequently happens, some animal species becomes very abundant,‘ the attacks of a great many species of flexible habits becomes concentrated upon it, and the numbers of the food-species are soon reduced to normal. Animals with non-specialized habits, by taking whatever food is easiest of access, act as regulators of disturbances within the association. A clear exposition of the manner in which species of generalized habits restore unbalanced conditions to equilibrium is given in a paper by Forbes (1883), in which the regulative action of birds upon insect oscillations is discussed. The animal’s status within the association is deter- mined not only by its food-habits, but by the sum-total of its physiological and behavior characters (its mores). The degree of dominance is indicated not merely by the degree of specialization of food-habits, but in all habits, by the degree of flexibility of behavior. An extreme specialization in nearly any behavior character, as habit of abode in the pit-digging ant-lion larva, prevents the species from becoming dominant. The degree of spe- cialization of behavior is thus a convenient criterion of the relative influence of animals in the association. The dominant animals are moderately specialized, and carry on the ordinary work of the association. The highly spe- cialized animals make use of space otherwise unoccupied and food material not demanded by other species. Cer- tain of the first group, with habits more highly regulatory than is usual, with perhaps some few unspecialized forms 4 With some animals sudden abundance is a matter of seasonal periodic- ity, as in the case of May-flies (Hexagenia) along the Illinois River (E:17). The adults on emerging become a sudden source of food for animals of ad- joining terrestrial associations, as the bunch-grass. 428 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (VoL. XLVIII in addition, tend, by following the path of least resist- ance, to act in opposition to forces tending to destroy the biotic equilibrium. IV. DISTRIBUTION WITHIN THE ASSOCIATION The association may be subdivided into minor groups of organisms, both in space and in time. Each group, being thus removed from the immediate influence of the others, is to some extent self-contained, having its own environmental conditions, its own assemblage of organ- isms, and its own system of interrelations. A. DISTRIBUTION IN Space® Different parts of the space occupied by an association present different environmental conditions. In the ver- tical distribution, four strata, the air (cf. E: 73), the plant layer, the surface layer and the underground layer, are usually present. In forest associations, the plant layer is complex, plants of various heights giving rise to minor strata (cf. A). In grassland associations the plant layer is relatively uniform. Animals are most numerous, during the feeding activity, in the plant layer. Others find food at the surface or underground. Many of the animals in the air or on the ground move about rapidly from plant to plant. Predaceous animals (while active) are frequently permanent members of air and ground layers, depending for food upon the transient animals and upon members of their own group. The ground stratum is composed of the surface and subsurface layers (E: 72), which are not, however, continuous horizontally, but alternate to greater or less extent. Local variability in horizontal distribution is due partly to local discontinuity of the various strata. This interruptedness is particularly conspicuous in open asso- ciations, where the plants do not form a dense growth, but are separated by open spaces. The subsurface area is provided by cover of various kinds, which lies more or less scattered about on the surface. 5 Cf. Shelford, A, B, 1912b, C; also D: 167; also p. — of this paper. No. 571] TERRESTRIAL ASSOCIATIONS 429 The motility of the animal allows change in stratum, and to some extent and in some animals, in habitat, with change in activity. The food-stratum and the food- habitat are apparently of greatest importance in the rela- tion of the animal to other organisms. B. DISTRIBUTION IN TIME Physiological activities of the plants are subject to diurnal variation, and are also greatly affected by varia- tions in weather conditions. The greater part of the animals of an association are active during the day. Others are nocturnal. During the inactive period of the day the animal rests in some more or less sheltered place, perhaps in a burrow or nest. The inactive state is also induced by unfavorable weather conditions. Seasonal changes in the association are very great in temperate climates, particularly in treeless regions, where the winters are severe. Seasonal changes in the vegetation are marked, certain groups of the plants appearing in successive periods during a sum- mer season, giving four or five successive aspects to the plant cover. A corresponding seasonal distribution is observed among the animals of the association (cf. D: 175). Annual changes in the associations are indicated by the very marked differences in the numbers of indi- viduals, in certain species of plants and animals, in suc- cessive years. This may be due (1) to fluctuation in the numerical adjustment between different organisms, and (2) to the effect of annually varying phenological condi- tions upon the various organisms. Oscillatory irregularities in the association take place at indefinite intervals. The causes and nature of oscilla- tions have been thoroughly treated in several of Forbes’s writings (1880, 1883, 1887). - V. INTERDEPENDENCE OF TERRESTRIAL PLANT AND ANIMAL COMMUNITIES The thesis of the following section is that, in terrestrial climatic or extensive environments, the relations between 430 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVII the assemblage of plants and the assemblage of animals are intimate and regular of occurrence; so much so that (1) the two are coextensive, (2) the two constitute to- gether a community which may be called a biotic asso- ciation, (3) neither plant nor animal assemblage usually occurs independently of the other, (4) the geographic distribution of many of the plant and animal species which make up the assemblages are in general corre- spondence, (5) the species composition of the association, over its range, varies no more widely, relatively speak- ing, than would an assemblage of plants alone. Perhaps the single view-point of the botanist, on one hand, and the zoologist, on the other, has tended to a neglect of the dual character of the one problem. Probably most botan- ists and zoologists agree that relations of animals and plants within a habitat are most intimate, and there is a tacit assumption that all the organisms in one place constitute the true system of interrelations, but botanists have spoken of plant communities, and zoologists of animal communities. There are numerous disharmonies and variations in agreement of plant and animal assem- blages, but these must not be allowed to obscure general facts of correspondence. It is recognized that plants and animals of an area of essentially homogeneous physical conditions are inter- dependent, the animals as a group being wholly depend- ent upon the plants for food, and many of the plants being directly dependent upon animals, as in the matter of pollination. All are directly or indirectly affected by animals in some way. It is also recognized that the plants are a good index to conditions for animal life, the plant assemblage affecting animals locally in modifica- tion of the physical environment, and more directly in providing food, shelter, ete. (4: 601). It is further ac- cepted that plants and animals respond to general en- vironmental conditions in similar manner (Craig, 1908). Thus considered, the character of the plant population of an area is an index to general character, or ecological No. 571] _ TERRESTRIAL ASSOCIATIONS 431 type, of the animal assemblage. These relations, how- ever, are quite general, lacking detail. Detailed consid- erations may be geographic, including geographic range of species and of communities, and the distribution of species and of individuals into communities; and they may also be local, dealing with interrelations of plants and animals within the area of the community. A. QEOGRAPHIC RELATIONS OF TERRESTRIAL PLANTS AND ÅNIMALS 1. Geographic Range: The Province.—If one were to plot the geographic range of the plant species found to- gether in a given climatic habitat, a general correspond- ence in distribution would be made apparent, a large number of the species ranging more or less continuously over a common, rather definite area (cf. Transeau, 1905). The similar ecological constitution of these plants and their consequent selective distribution into similar envi- ronmental complexes gives a uniformity to the vegeta- tion over the geographic region in which these environ- mental conditions are found, and the resulting vegeta- tion unit is known as a vegetation province (Gleason, 1910: 42). The area of the province is generally uniform in physical conditions. This uniformity is only relative, — being subject to gradual geographic variation in climate, perhaps giving rise to subregions in distant parts of the province, and to abrupt local variations in soil, water- content, exposure, etc., giving rise to local or edaphic plant assemblages very different from those of the cli- matic or geographic type. Thus the prairie province occupies the winter-dry interior region of North America. Environmental variations from east to west, climatic and physiographic, divide the province into the three sub- regions of Pound and Clements (1898). Certain plant species range over one or all of these subregions, still others establishing themselves over the whole area of the province and also scatteringly eastward, in dry treeless parts of the deciduous forest province, to the Atlantic coast. These last are also typical prairie plants, though 432 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [VoL. XLVIII extra-limital in parts of an adjoining province locally approximating the prairie environment. The habitat-selection of different animal species re- sults, in precisely the same manner, in similarity of geo- graphic range among ecologically similar animals. These correspondences of distribution point to the existence of definite areas characterized by general similarity of the animal assemblages. As the physical factors of the en- vironment are the same ultimately for animals as for plants, and as the vegetational environment for animals has the same range as the physical environment, we might expect animal communities to have the same geo- graphic distribution as plant communities, and we might expect the area of the plant province to be characterized by distinctive kinds of animals as well as by distinctive kinds of plants. The province is thus not simply a vege- tation province, but a biotic province. This is not a new notion. Ruthven (1908: 388-390) has stated a cur- ` rent viewpoint as follows: Those who are acquainted with the literature of the field zoology of North America are familiar with the fact that, since the time of the Pacific Railroad surveys, naturalists have noted that there are in North America well-defined biological regions. These have been pointed out at various times by Allen, Cope, Merriam, and others, and the fauna of each has been more or less investigated. . . . For example, we have forms of birds, reptiles and mammals characteristic of the southeastern de- ciduous forest region, and still others characteristic of the northeastern coniferous forest region, ete. Shelford (4: 604) bases his classification of animal regions upon that of plant regions, as worked out by Schimper (1903) and Transeau (1903, 1905). How close the correspondence of distribution of par- ticular animals with that of vegetation provinces may be, is well shown in the case of North American rabbits (Nelson, 1909). The distribution maps shown for certain species and groups of these animals might almost serve as maps of the provinces. Many other animals, verte- brate and invertebrate, correspond in area with the plant provinces. Among the insects listed by Hart (1907: 205) No. 571] ‘TERRESTRIAL ASSOCIATIONS 433 as western species, those for which a number of locality records are available are plainly to be assigned to the prairie province, the range of most of them extending west to the Rocky Mountains, north about as far as Montana, east to Illinois or Indiana, and south to Texas. Other animal species bear apparently no relation to province boundaries. Such animals have been discussed by Shelford (A: 606, footnote), who shows them to be of three types: (1) Species of scattered but very wide range, covering perhaps several plant realms (animals of local associations of extreme habitats); (2) Species occupying only a part of the plant realm in which they belong (animals of such ecological constitution that their range is restricted by some conditions unfavorable in certain parts of the province); (3) Species occupying intermediate ground between two realms—these are few (Ruthven). These exceptional species are found also in plants, so that local associations are occupied by both plants and animals of the secattered-but-wide type of range, while certain subregions, as the Great Plains area of the prairie province, contain associations with both plant and animal species restricted to these less extensive areas. Associations of two adjoining provinces may inter- grade, if ecologically similar, or may alternate if dis- similar. Similar associations of two provinces may con- tain the same or closely related species, as with certain grasshoppers which range in both northeastern and west- ern coniferous provinces (D: 173). But these same asso- ciations contain also plant species in common, so that irregularities of range are no greater in animals than in plants. 2. Distribution Within the Province: Distribution of Plants and Animals into Communities.—It is seen that plant and animal species may correspond closely in geo- graphic range. There may be also more local corre- spondence in distribution. The plant community has been found by the writer to be the convenient index of the 434 ` THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vow. XLVII area of the habitat for animals. It has been observed, in an area in Michigan, that grasshopper species corre- spond closely in local distribution with plant communities (D). There is evidence that local distribution of ani- mals is seldom promiscuous as a result of motility (D: 159). It appears also that the local variability of envi- ronmental conditions within the area of the climatic plant community is sufficiently great, usually, to supply all necessary conditions for a large number of animals, so that the limits of the plant community need not be passed, ordinarily. The animal community of the area may be thus, in large measure, self- contained, and coextensive with the plant community (D : 161). One of the problems of plant ecology has been the differentiation of plant communities or associations. Mere comparison of lists of species is not sufficient; rela- tive abundance of various species must be considered as well. Animal assemblages in contiguous areas must be separated in the same way. Given two adjoining habitats differing in plant population, it has been found that, in addition to differences of animal species, there are also differences of relative abundance in those animal apoa common to the two areas (D: 154, 167). The local area of a plant community is determined by (1) local distribution of the physical environmental com- plex, and (2) influence (competition, etc.) of adjoining plant communities. Local area of the animal community depends upon (1) local distribution of physical environ- ment, and (2) local distribution of vegetational environ- ment, the latter being uniform over the area of the plant community. Contiguous areas differing in physical and vegetational conditions will be expected to differ also in animal population, in a degree comparable to that of the differences in environmental conditions. Physical habitats, and plant communities, sometimes alternate, sometimes intergrade; it is not unreasonable to expect accompanying alternation or intergradation of 6 Differences in species, both plant and animal, are accompanied by dif- ferences in ecological constitution. No. 571] TERRESTRIAL ASSOCIATIONS 435 animal populations. Certain of the animal assemblages of sand habitats, as studied in central Illinois, intergrade; others, as oak forest and bunch-grass, differ radically. The above considerations, if correct, appear to signify that, in ordinary climatic development of plant and ani- mal life in temperate land environments, the area of the animal assemblage is that of the plant assemblage, both resting basically upon the physical environment. The plant and animal assemblages are therefore coextensive parts of a biotic association, composed of both plants and animals, and this association as a whole constitutes the real terrestrial community of living organisms. B. Locat RELATIONS or PLANT AND ANIMAL ASSEMBLAGES (RELATIONS WITHIN THE ÅSSOCIATION) The more intimate relations between plants and ani- mals are seen in the detailed study of a single associa- tion. The bunch-grass association of sand prairie is selected for illustration (E: 68). 1. Similarity of Ecological Type of Plants and Ani- mals —Shelford has shown (A: 593-594) that animals and plants may evince ecological similarity by similar response to the same general environmental conditions, behavior responses in animals’ corresponding to struc- tural responses in plants,’ so that mores of the animal may be in accord with growth-form in the plant. Shel- ford states (B: 87) that ‘‘plants and animal communities are in full agreement when the growth-form of each stratum of the plant-community is correlated with the conditions selected by the animals of that stratum.’’ In the bunch-grass there is general agreement, ac- cording to this criterion. The herbaceous stratum is oc- cupied mainly by tuft and mat plants—bunch-grasses, cactus and a few half-shrubs. Associated with the tuft or mat growth-form is the sedentary mores of the plant- inhabiting animals (leaf-beetles, stem-borers, ambush- bugs, ete.). A considerable proportion of ground surface 7 Or motile organisms, ef. C: 305. 8 Or sessile organisms. 436 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII is bare sand; in the interspaces between the dominant plants are slender annuals (interstitial plants), and here are also found animals of the roving mores of the ground stratum (interstitial animals). Many of these are swift- running and predaceous (six-lined lizard, tiger ae lycosid spiders). Correspondence in ecological type of plants and ani- mals in the bunch-grass is not complete in several re- spects. Shelford mentions types of disagreement (B: 88; C: 306-308), and there is a further important kind of disharmony, in mixed associations, due to presence of diverse types of plants and animals (D: 163). Mixed associations are quite frequent in forest border regions, and in the transition area between two provinces. The plant and animal assemblages of a given habitat, partic- ularly if climatic and extensive, are usually in general ecological agreement, and the exceptions are likely to be infrequent or temporary (Shelford, B: 88). 2. Relative Dependence of Plant and Animal Assem- blages——There is evidence that the agreement of plant and animal assemblages of terrestrial associations is often a matter of accommodation on the part of the animal assemblage. In the early stages of development of vegetation, local physical conditions dominate; in later stages the vegetation assumes the type determined by climatic conditions, and exerts nearly complete con- trol over local physical factors. In established associa- tions, therefore, the locally dominating environmental feature is the vegetation. Shelford states that in the several associations of a successional series, the domi- nating animal mores are correlated with the dominating conditions (B: 94) and that, as the forest increases in density, the animals make use of the vegetation in in- creasing degree, particularly for breeding-places, and as places of abode (B: 90). Many grasshoppers of open grassland depend upon a particular kind of soil for egg- laying, while those of closed forest lay eggs in fallen logs—a condition of the plant environment (D: 163). No. 571] TERRESTRIAL ASSOCIATIONS 437 The sand-prairie vegetation is in an intermediate stage, certain animals depending chiefly on the presence of loose bare sand, others on the bunch-grass vegetation. With development of bunch-grass into closed grassland, the interstitial animals are eliminated. The animals of established associations, while in accord with climatic physical conditions, are perhaps more intimately affected by vegetation conditions. Since established associations are very much more extensive than primitive associa- tions, the importance of vegetation as a dominating part of the environment for animals becomes apparent, and we may conclude that the character of the plant assem- blage determines, to a large extent, the ecological type of the animal assemblage. 3. Correspondence in Distribution within the Associa- tion.—The uniformity of physical and vegetational con- ditions is only relative. There are spots in the bunch- grass association in which local invasion of blue grass has occurred, darkening and binding the soil. In such partly humified situations, small colonies of the corn- field ant, not occurring elsewhere in the bunch-grass (E: 57), have been found. There are also areas some few feet in diameter in which the bunches of grass are few, small and seattered. In these relatively bare patches the abundance of interstitial animals is greatly increased. More direct relations are seen in the case of animals associated with particular species of plants. Within the association, any animal species, like any plant species, may be distributed generally throughout the area, or it may be restricted to a part of the area characterized by a slight environmental difference, or it may occur in seattered parts of the association, characterized by seattered local differences (D: 168). There is evidence that, in so far as the vegetational environment is con- cerned, distribution of animals within the association is usually a direct function of similar distribution of plants. 4. Uniformity of Species Composition of Plant and Animal Assemblages.—It has been seen that plant assem- 438 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou, XLVII blages of definite ecological type, as regards growth- form, etc., are regularly accompanied by animal assem- blages of similar ecological type, as regards mores. In- terest attaches also to the problem whether associated plant and animal assemblages show definite species relations. — One familiar with a certain association, who visits a representation of that same growth in a different part of the same climatic region, will be struck with the fact that a large proportion of both plant and animal species is well known, while a certain proportion, perhaps con- siderably smaller, is new to him. The writer has been impressed with the similarity of the plant and animal populations of the sandhills of central Nebraska and of eastern Colorado, to those of the sand prairie of central and western Illinois, despite the fact that certain species are not common to the two areas. Tiger-beetles, blow- snake, grasshoppers, box-turtle, six lined lizard, western meadow-lark, white-footed mouse, among the animals; prickly-pear, lead-plant, bunch-grasses, sand-bur, sand evening primrose, among the plants; are represented in the two areas either by the same or by closely related varieties and species. There are no yuccas or sand-sages in the Illinois sand prairie, no lizard Holbrookia nor lubber-grasshopper Brachystola; and there are certain eastern species not found in the western sandhills. But on the whole the species (particularly the important species) common to the two areas are more numerous. This is the more remarkable in view of the fact that dis- tribution of sand prairie is discontinuous, the largest, nearly uninterrupted gap being several hundred miles in extent. Many of the animals, as well as plant species, of dry mixed prairie-grass in loamy soil, are the same along the mountain-front in Colorado (Vestal, 1914b) as in north-central Illinois. The likenesses become much more impressive as distance is decreased. Absolute identity of species composition, where large numbers of species are involved, is an ideal condition, No. 571] TERRESTRIAL ASSOCIATIONS 439 never actually attained. No one can say just what pro- portion of species-in-common is necessary for two growths to be’said to represent the same association. In addition to likenesses and differences of environment, of aspect, and of history, which must be weighed, the differ- ent plant and animal species vary so much in importance in the association, in physiological variation and in range of environmental tolerance, that associations can hardly be separated or placed together on a statistical basis. A comparison of species is fair if the following kinds of plants and animals are left out of consideration; (1) those of limited range within the climatic region or prov- ince, including species belonging more properly to other provinces; (2) those of very indefinite habitat-relations, which are found in nearly any kind of habitat; (3) those of special restricted habitats, which may be scattered about in many kinds of associations, as moist dead wood, in which particular fungi, beetles, perhaps snails, myrio- pods and pill-bugs, are usually found; or as excrement of grazing animals, in which certain molds, certain dipter- ous and scarabeid larve, etc., regularly occur, irrespect- ive of surrounding conditions; (4) invaders from near- by associations; (5) ruderal and introduced species; and possibly one or two other groups. The second and third groups may be called the irregular element; the fourth and fifth may be known as the derived element. While these groups make a formidable list, their representa- tives constitute usually a very small proportion of the organisms of the association. The other organisms, and some of these, follow habitat-differences, as represented in different associations, very closely. Since hardly any two species are identical in habitat- relations, geographic and even local variation must be looked for, but since many species resemble each other more or less closely in general ecological relations, there come to be recognized certain ecological groups of spe- cies, each characterized by a general type of growth-form in plants, or by a general kind of mores in animals, and 440 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII these groups may be considered to be small or large, according as we emphasize minor differences or general likenesses. Now within any limited region (let us postulate first an area removed from the influence of an adjoining prov- ince) there are only a limited number of ecological groups, of growth-forms of plants, and of mores among animals, each group represented by a limited collection of species. Each habitat within this restricted area will be characterized by definite physical conditions, and with these will be correlated certain growth-forms of plants and certain mores of animals, each represented by as many of the species as can migrate into and survive within the area, as determined first by capabilities of migration and by habitat-selection, and second by inter- relation of species and of individuals. It follows that physical complexes which are alike will become populated with similar complexes of ecological groups, represented by similar collections of plant and animal species, and that unlike physical areas will be occupied by different combinations of ecological groups, and will be composed of different species. Two areas within this region which have similar physical conditions and similar plant growths will be expected to have a large number of ani- mal species in common, although direct relations between species of animals and species of plants obtain only rarely (between comparatively few associated plant-and- animal pairs). It is to be noted that species composition of the animal assemblage varies proportionately no more widely than does that of the plant assemblage. No terrestrial continental region is sufficiently isolated to be free from influence of surrounding areas, and since the influences are different from different directions, and since there is continual change of physical conditions, and of range and abundance of plant and animal species, there must be more or less local and geographic varia- tion of species composition within similar but separated habitats. Geographic variation is wider with distance. No. 571] TERRESTRIAL ASSOCIATIONS 441 because the geographic and physiographic complexes vary geographically, as well as the entire collection of plant and animal species which may invade the habitat. Within the area of the climatic province, however, or at least within the area of a subregion of the province, climatic, physiographic and biotic complexes are likely to be relatively constant, that is, likenesses of two areas are likely to be greater and more striking than differ- ences. Within the province or subregion, therefore, it is to be expected that species composition of association of closely similar habitats will be relatively constant. Particular plant and animal assemblages will be found together, both associated with a particular habitat. Field observation bears out these expectations. Conditions within the transition zone between two climatie regions or provinces are much more complex than in an area in the middle of a sub-region or province; climatic and physiographic conditions vary to wider extremes and are less stable; the total number of species near enough at hand to invade a given habitat is much greater. Mixed associations, often transitional as re- gards physical conditions, are composed of representa- tives of both provinces. Animals of a particular associa- tion of one province, may be found with plants of a similar or equivalent association of the other province. When three geographic elements are represented, as at the southern end of Lake Michigan (cf. C, and Vestal, 1914a), the complication of conditions is extreme. Even here, on the dry sand of old lake beaches, fairly typical representations of sand prairie can be seen; and though such habitats are shared with deciduous forest associa- tions, and with associations of the northeastern coniferous forest province, and with mixed associations, the bunch- grass growth can still be recognized in dry shifting sterile sand, with bunch grass plant species, and bunch-grass animal species. The tendency towards uniformity of association of plant and animal assemblages is even here to be made out. 442 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII If the foregoing considerations relating to relations between plant and animal communities are correct, the thesis mentioned at the beginning of part V would seem to be justified, though the evidence is far from complete. Plant and animal assemblages are mutually interdepend- ent; the plant assemblage dominates in established associations. Plant and animal assemblages correspond in geographic distribution, in distribution into commu- nities, and in more detailed distribution within the habitat. They are made up of ecologically similar groups correlated with the same physical conditions or with ‘each other. Though there are few direct relations be- tween particular species of plants and animals, it so happens that within any restricted region, particular collections of animal species come into regular associa- tion with particular collections of plant species, the spe- cies composition within the habitat exhibiting a greater or less degree of uniformity, except for minor irregular and derived elements. The more restricted, or uniform in biological conditions, this region is, the greater the uniformity of the collection of species. Climatic and ex- tensive associations, and established associations, show a greater degree of uniformity than local or primitive associations, VI. SUMMARY AND CONCLUSIONS The discussion is based principally upon the writer’s study of prairie associations, the bunch-grass associa- tion of sand prairie in Illinois being chiefly used for illus- tration. Internal activities of the association are a com- plex of activities of all the organisms. Environmental influences are of three classes, physical, plant and animal. The characters of plants and animals are interpreted in their relation to these influences. Characters of plants may be classed as structural, physiological, biographical and numerical. Animals have, in addition, behavior or psychological characters. These groups of characters are intimately related, one to another. The relations of the animal to its animal-environment are of two kinds, No. 571] TERRESTRIAL ASSOCIATIONS 443 social and antagonistic, the latter relations being with food-species, competitors and enemies. Correlations of the various kinds of characters with relations involving food, competition and enemies, are given. According to its ecological constitution, each organism finds a status in the association, the whole being a self-contained and self-regulating system of activities. Dependencies within the association are concerned mainly with sources and interchange of material and energy. Dominant plants (the most influential species) are those most intimately correlated with physical en- vironment, as indicated by aggressiveness, abundance, frequence, size, ete. Dominant animals are most numer- ous among phytophagous forms. Dominance in an ani- mal species includes dependence of other animals upon it (for food) plus the ability to thrive in spite of the drain upon its numbers. The degree of specialization of be- havior is a convenient index of the relative influence of animals in the association. The dominant animals are moderately specialized, and carry on the ordinary work of the association. The highly specialized animals make use of space otherwise unoccupied, and food material not available to other species, or not taken by other forms. Least highly specialized animals act as a check upon undue departure from biotic equilibrium. The association may be divided into minor groups of organisms, both in space and in time. Space-division is vertical, resulting in strata, and horizontal, resulting in sub-habitats of greater or less magnitude. The strata and sub-habitats present a larger or smaller degree of discontinuity and of internal variability. Time-distribu- tion is diurnal, seasonal and annual. There are also time-variations produced by variability of weather condi- tions and by oscillatory disturbances. The relations between plant and animal assemblages have long been known, in a general way, to be intimate. Plants and animals agree in similar response to common environmental influence, and in types of geographic dis- 444 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII tribution. Upon investigation, it begins to appear that plant and animal assemblages are coextensive parts of a biotic association, composed of both plants and animals, and this association as a whole constitutes the real ter- restrial community of living organisms. Plant and ani- mal assemblages are mutually interdependent; the plant assemblage dominates in established associations. Plant and animal assemblages correspond in geographic dis- tribution, in distribution into communities, and in more detailed distribution within the habitat. They are made up of ecologically similar groups correlated with the same physical conditions or with each other. Though there are few direct relations between particular species of plants and animals, it so happens that within any restricted region, particular collections of animal species come into regular association with particular collections of plant species, the species composition within the habi- tat exhibiting a greater or less degree of uniformity, except for minor irrégular or derived elements. The more restricted in area, or uniform in biological condi- tions, this region is, the greater uniformity of the collec- tion of species. Climatic and extensive associations show a higher degree of uniformity than local or primitive associations. VII. REFERENCES Special References (A) mugen Vv. 0% 11b. ae ae Animal Geography. Jour. Morya: 22: 551- (B) 1912a, Ecological Succession. IV. Vegetation and the Control of Land Animal Communities. Biol. Bull., 23: 59-99 (C) 1913. Animal Communities in Temperate America. Geogr. Soc. of Chicago, Bull. No. 5, p. 362. Chicago. (D) Vestal,. A. G. 13a. REF Distribution of Grasshoppers in Relation to Plant ssociations. Biol. Bull., 25: 141-180. CE a nas 1913b. An Associational Study of Illinois Sand Prairie. Bull. Ill. State Lab. Nat. Hist., 10: 1-96. Other Articles Cited Adams, C. C. 1913. Guide to the Study of Animal Ecology, p. 183. New York. No. 571] TERRESTRIAL ASSOCIATIONS 445 Craig, W. 1908. eee Dakota Life: Plant, Animal, and Human. Bull. Am. gr. Koc., 40: 321-332, 401-415. Ellis, M. M. 1914. Fishes of Colorado. Univ. Colo. Studies, 11: 1-136. Forbes, S. A. 1880. On Some aas of Organisms. Bull. IU. State Lab. Nat. Hist., Vol. 1, No. 3, pp. 3-17 1883. The Benais Action of Birds upon Insect waa ce Bull. Ill. State Lab. Nat. His., Vol. 1, No. 6, pp. 3 1887. The Lake as a a Reprint from Bull Sei. Assoc. of Peoria, Il., pp. 1-15. 1909. The General bien Ecology of the Indian Corn Plant. Am. Nat., 43: 286-301. Gates, F. C. 1912. The Vegetation of the Beach Area in Northeastern Illinois and Southeastern Wisconsin. Bull. IU. State Lab. Nat. Hist., 9: 55-372 Gleason, H. A. 1910. The Vegetation of the Inland Sand PEMP of Illinois. Bull. Ill. State Lab. Nat. Hist., 9: 23-1 pees C. A., and Gleason, H. A. 190 ‘On the cones of the Sand tomas of Illinois. Bull. Ill. State Lab. Nat. Hist., 7: 137-2 Nelson, E. W. 1909. The Rabbits of North pei Bur. Biol. Surv., U. S. Dept. Agr., N. Am. ea No. 29, pp. 314. pe R., and Clements 1898. The Varant ait of the Prairie Province. Bot. Gaz., 25: 381-394. Ruthven, A. G. 1908. The Faunal Affinities of yf ae Region of Central North merica. AM. NAT., 42: 388-393. Schimper, A. F. 1903. Plant pen upon a Physiological Basis. Shelford, V. E. 1911a. Ecological Succession. I. Stream Fishes and the Method of hysiographie repens eee Biol. Bull., 21: 9-35. 1912b. Ecological Succession. V. Aspects of Physiological Classifi- eation. Biol. Bull., 23: 331-370. Oxford. Vestal, A. G. 1914a. The Status of Prairie oe in the pap Beach Areas of Lake Michigan. r. Ecology. (In press.) 1914. Prairie Vegetation of a a front Area in | Colorado. Bot. Gaz. (In press Transeau, E. N.. i 1903. On the Geographie Distribution and Ecological Relation of Bog Plant Societies. Bot. Gaz., 36: 40 seq. 1905. Forest Centers of Eastern heiatins: Am. Nart., 39: 875-889. SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION ANOTHER HYPOTHESIS TO ACCOUNT FOR DR. SWINGLE’S EXPERIMENTS WITH CITRUS THE results of the cross-breeding experiments with forms of Citrus by Walter Swingle have given rise to quite a number of different hypotheses, to account for the facts observed. The facts are simply these. All the different.forms of Citrus used in the experiments, Citrus trifoliata, the lemon, orange and other citrous fruits have, so far, proved to reproduce their own type through seed. Nevertheless, the plants raised from one single cross are ex- ceedingly different among themselves. And yet, all these new forms, for so far as tested, have proved truly to reproduce their own kind only, if sown. The theories offered to account for PEN facts are rather com- plex. So far, we have not seen the simple hypothesis which we want to add to the others. The fact, that the F, from almost every cross between types of Citrus is multiform, can only be accounted for on the assumption, that the parent plants are impure (heterozygous) for a number of genes. The difficult question is this: how can a tree, impure for a number of genes, produce seed which always only repro- duces the type? We know, that if a plant reproduces itself by an asexual method, all its daughter plants are pure for those genes in respect to which it was pure, impure for those genes for which it was impure. Is it possible that in these trees the seeds normally produced are not derived from a union between two normal gametes? In Citrus, with its adventitious embryos, this is very well possible. If the forms of Citrus used by Dr. Swingle are self-sterile, the seeds normally produced by these trees, are not produced by the union of two gametes, but as buds, asexually. This hypothesis, that the Citrus used are self-sterile, and that the seeds normally produced, are produced asexually, fully ac- counts for all the facts. All the daughter plants from un- crossed seeds are genotypically identical with the mother plant, as in all clones. On pollenization by another tree, normal se are produced, each the result of the union of two real gametes. 446 ` : No.d571] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 447 These seeds contain different combinations of the genes, for which the parent plants are impure, as normally. The F, gener- ation for this reason becomes as diverse as such generations always are, if the parents are impure for numerous genes. But these daughter plants, although impure for a number of genes, can, because of their self-sterility, in their turn only pro- duce seed asexually and therefore their offspring will be like themselves, It should not be difficult to test our hypothesis. It seems easier to find out, whether the seeds produced without crossing in Citrus contain the embryo formed by fertilization of the em- bryo sac, or embryos formed adventitiously by the adjacent tis- sue, than to test any of the other theories, which assume a pecul- iar behavior of the chromosomes. Our hypothesis, that a variable F,, of only true-breeding plants (from the union of two true-breeding forms), results from habitual self-sterility and asexual production of seed, with real fertilization in the case of a cross taking place, not only accounts for the facts found by Swingle, but also for those found by Rosen with Erophila verna. These facts were somewhat dif- ferent. The F, plants were all identical, and somewhat inter- mediate. They gave rise to a variable F, generation of which all the plants bred true to their type. These facts can be ex- plained on the assumption, that Erophila verna is self-sterile, and that, in the absence of cross-fertilization, unfertilized egg- cells develop parthenogenetically. Such F, plants, which are impure for a number of genes, should therefore produce as many different kinds of F, plants, as there are female gametes pro- duced, and in the same proportions. In the case of such a plant being impure for two genes, we should expect it to produce plants of the four different types, not in the usual proportion of 9:3:3:1, but in equal proportions, 1:1:1:1. The F, plants from such seed could only be pure for all the genes present. It would be possible in Erophila verna to find out whether F, plants, impure for two genes, produced daughter plants of the four kinds, AB, Ab, aB, and ab, in the proportion of 9:3:3:1, or in proportion 1:1:1:1, and thus to test our hypothesis. To find out, whether it is possible, that a plant, impure for a _ number of genes, produces a variable F, generation of only completely homozygous plants, we have begun a series of experi- ments with squashes. Some hybrid plants have not produced a 448 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIIT single fruit from carefully sealed female buds, others have given plenty of empty fruit, but some hybrids have produced several fruits, full of viable seed. If this seed is formed by the par- thenogenetic development of unfertilized normal egg-cells, as we have reason to believe, we expect to raise a variable F, gene- ration of exclusively homozygous plants. If these seeds have developed by apogamy, or any other asexual process, we expect to obtain a second generation consisting exclusively of plants like the original hybrids. Thus we will have a non-cytological test to decide between apogamy and true parthenogenesis. A. C. HAGEDOORN, A. L. HAGEDOORN Bussum, HOLLAND, March 18, 1914 oh VOL. XLVIII, NO. 572 K AUGUST, 1914 THE AMERICAN NATURALISE A MONTHLY JOURNAL Devoted to the Advancement of the Biological Sciences with Special Reference to the Factors of Evolution CONTENTS Page I. Multiple Allelomorphs in Mice. Professor T. H. MORGAN ~ ~ -~ - 449 II. Thirteen Years of Wheat Selection. T. B. HUTCHESON - - = 469 OI. Pattern Development in Mammals and eae GLOVER M. ALLEN - - 467 IV. The Meadow Jumping Mouse. Dr. H. L. BABCOCK = s - 485 V. Shorter Articles and Discussion: nak on ee Dr. RAYMOND PEARL. Parallel Mutations in (nothera biennis L. Dr. J. 8tomps, Dr BRADLEY M. Davis. The Theoretical ne as seers Multiple Allelo- nates nd Close —— TRETE. T. H , Professor W. E. — 491 VI. Pipers and p ee Monetti YMOND p Paani. i New Mode of Segregation in Gregory’s Tetraploid ai HERMANN J. MULLER ~ 505 THE SCIENCE PRESS LANCASTER, PA. GARRISON, N. F. NEW YORE: SUB-STATION 84 The American Naturalist intended for inte and books, etc., intended for review should be MSS sent to _ ae of THEA ICAN NATURALIST, Garrison- -on-Hudson, New York. rticles containing summaries of research work bear ring on the problems of organic evolution are especialiy welcome, and will be given preference in sng canoes reprints of contributions are supplied -to authors free of charge. Further 1 reprints will be supplied a cos criptions and sivertiesments should be sent to the Larong ar. Foreign postage sclisvigiion price is four dollars a Canadian postage twenty-five cents additional. The s fifty cents and The eho for single copies is forty cents. The advertising rates are Four Dollars for a p THE SCIENCE PRESS NEW YORK: Sub-Station 84 Entered as second-class matter, April 2, 1908, at the Post Office at Lancaster, Pa., under the Act of ongress of March 3, 1879. Lancaster, Pa. Garrison, N. Y. F E ARCTIC, ICELAND and GREENLAND BIRDS’ SKINS, Well cage Low Prices articulars of _ DINESEN. Bird Collecto Husavik, ‘North Iceland, Via Leidle, England WANTED TO PURCHASE a set of BIRDS OF AMERICA by J. J. Audubon, 7 or 8 volumes, please report, stating cash price, stat- ing condition, binding and dates of volumes. F. C. HARRIS, Box 2244 Boston, Massachusetts For Sale Entire An important collection of Indian Birds’ Eggs, containing approximately 3,300 specimens of about 620 species, and in- cluding among other varieties the Green- ish Willow Warbler (Acanthopneuste viridanus), Red-browed Finch (Calla- eanthis burtoni), Tibetan Sand-Grouse (Syrrhaptes tibetanus), Spotted Sand- Grouse (Pteroclurus senegallus), Ibis- bill (ibidorhynchus struthersi), White- bellied Minivet (Pericrocotus erythro- pygius), Bearded Vulture (Gypaétus barbatus), numerous Cuckoos with their hosts, ete., ete. Particulars and full list may be had rom W. F. H. ROSENBERG 57 Haverstock Hill London, N. W. Marine Biological Laboratory Woods Hole, Mass. INVESTIGATION Entire Year ue for research in Z for rie ip a > table is laboratory on £ Comps Pet ures are offered in ao INSTRUCTION July 1 to August 11, Beoloey a of grey Phy wad and Bog Vi stati tion. Each o the full time of te “student E Fee $50.00. A lectu uree io rg wig ne “Aspects of of mi UPPLY Animals S DEPARTMENT and ts, pre living, and embryonic ol Preserved material of all typet ip Open the Entire Year animals and of Algae, Fungi, ed for wo Mosses furnish aah classwork, or for the, muse, Living material furnished in serasi ordered. ice lists no torial logical and Bı cal pT sent on application. State i all is desired. For prios 1 obe iiai, information regarding GEO. M. GRAY, Curator, Woods Hole, Mass The annual announcement will be sent on application to | The Director, Marine Biological Laboratory, Woods | Mass- THE AMERICAN NATURALIST VoL. XLVIII August, 1914 No. 572 MULTIPLE ALLELOMORPHS IN MICE PROFESSOR T. H. MORGAN COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY Some breeding experiments with mice that I have been carrying on during the last two years have shown that yellow, gray gray-belly, gray white-belly and black are allelomorphs. To this series a fifth allelomorph may pos- - Sibly be added which for the present may be called new gray. This quadruple (or quintuple) system of allelo- -morphs fulfils the conditions of a multiple series in that only two of the allelomorphs can exist at the same time in any individual. In other words, a mouse may be pure for any of these genes (except for yellow, in which the pure form is not viable), or a mouse may be heterozygous in any two of the genes, but never in more than two. The evidence that establishes this series of allelomorphs may be briefly stated as follows: In 1911, I pointed out that if yellow mice (producing -yellow and chocolates) are bred to agoutis (grays), and their yellow offspring mated, they should produce not only yellow and agoutis (as they did) but some choco- lates (or blacks) also; but no chocolates appeared. I stated that the results obtained were explicable if yellow and agouti are allelomorphs.! 1 The discussion in the same paper of the presence of chocolate yellow and black bars in the ticked hair in relation to the occurrence of chocolate, yel- . low and black color in domesticated races may only confuse the ontogenetic production of characters with the gametie inheritance of factors. The 449 450 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII Sturtevant (1912) showed that the results are also consistent with the hypothesis that there is close or com- plete linkage (genetic coupling) between yellow and agouti. In principle this is the same as saying that when yellow and agouti enter from different sides (mother and father) they separate in gametogenesis, or in other words they ‘‘repel’’ each other and behave, as I said, like allelomorphs. The numerical results would be the same whether yellow and agouti are treated as though completely linked or whether they are treated as allelomorphic. What I had vaguely seen in my 1911 paper was clearly explained in the following year by Sturtevant’s treat- ment of the same data, to which he added that of Little and Miss Durham. Sturtevant showed, from an analysis of Miss Dur-. ham’s results, in which she used ordinary gray (gray ‘‘oray-belly’’) mice, that her results are consistent with the hypothesis of absolute linkage, or, on my interpre- tation, with the hypothesis of allelomorphism. Sturte- vant’s conclusions were promptly contradicted by C. C. Little on the evidence furnished by some of his earlier experiments, in which he obtained yellow, grays and black (or chocolates) in offspring from yellow to black (or chocolates). Such a result would be inconsistent with Sturtevant’s hypothesis. Little also appealed to certain experiments of Miss Durham, in which, he stated, results like his own are given. Since Little has been unable to get again his former results, but has obtained evidence in favor of Sturtevant’s view, and since it is clear that he misunderstood Miss Durham’s evidence, his contradiction ceases to have any weight. factorial hypothesis relates to those differentials that serve to separate different types in inheritance and is not concerned with the problem as to how those differentials produce their effects. Breeding experiments show that gray differs from black by one differential, from yellow by another, and from cinnamon by a third. So far as Mendelian segregation of these dif- ferential genes is concerned it is es no consequence that the gray hair is made up of a black, a yellow, and a chocolate band. No. 572] MULTIPLE ALLELOMORPHS 451 After the publication of my own and of Sturtevant’s paper I set to work to obtain crucial evidence in favor of, or opposed to, the view that yellow and gray are allelomorphic. Little, also, it appears, has carried out some new experiments which he has recently published, with the results just stated. My own data have been _ ready for some time, but I have withheld them in order to get a sufficient body of evidence to make the case con- vincing, especially in the light of the possibility that the crossing over might occur in one sex and not in the other. For, if no crossing over occurred in the male, there might be crossing over in the other sex, which would not reveal itself unless the experiments were deliberately planned so that both sexes are tested. This consideration seems to have been overlooked by Little, for he has omitted in his confirmatory paper to give the sexes of the animals used. Without a knowledge of this relation even his confirmation fails to confirm (as he supposes) the view that he formerly combated. Since Miss Durham worked with common gray and I with gray white-belly, and both are ‘‘repelled’’ by yellow, i. e., both are allelomorphs of yellow, it follows that these two grays are also allelomorphic to each other. The evidence that black belongs to the same series of allelomorphs is obtained in the following way: If a given yellow is mated to black, and yellow and gray offspring are obtained, and if then the yellow offspring are mated to black again and now give yellow and black only, the proof is furnished; for in the first mating yellow and agouti have repelled each other, and the yellow-bearing gametes have united with the black gametes of the other sex to give the yellow offspring. The second mating shows that black is now repelled in turn by yellow and is therefore allelomorphic. This may be illustrated in the following way: Let BY — yellow, b= black and B = gray. These three factors may be treated as allelomorphs, then: 452 . THE AMERICAN NATURALIST — [Vou. XLVIII Yellow B'B by | black bb. Gametes of P, yellow BY-B, Gametes of P, black b—b. F BYb = yellow. 1 7 Gametes of F, yellow B-b. -~ Gametes 5 _— black b-b Yb = yellow. Fy bb = black, But if yellow and black and gray are not allelomorphic the same matings should ie the following results: Y’ = yellow. y’=not yellow = "i gray’? (not black). Yellow Y’y 'BB or. Sng PAS Gametes of P, yellow Y’B-B. Gametes of P, pure black y'b-y’ b. F Y’B y’b = yellow. 2 yB yb = gray. Gametes of F, yellow Y’b—Y’B-y’b-y’B. Gametes of pure black y’b-y’b. F, yB y’b=gra On the second assumption yellow, gray and black should appear in the back cross. The former and not the latter view is therefore consistent with the actual results. THE SYMBOLS EMPLOYED It is, of course, a matter of secondary importance what system of symbols is followed. The requirements are simplicity, consistency and suggestiveness, but one can not always arrange to have all three at the same time. The simplest scheme, for a system of allelomorphs like these, would be to have some common letter to indicate their relation and an exponent to suggest the different characters for which each stands. If we take the symbol b (black) for the common letter, and use capitals for dominance, the allelomorphs will be: b- = bink. a pny y. BW = gray white belly. BY = yellow i one preferred to take Y Kaw as the common letter the series would be y’, y”, y'", Y’; or, if one preferred No. 572] MULTIPLE ALLELOMORPHS ` 453 to take G (gray), as the common letter, the series would be g’, g", G, g”. On the whole the first series seems to me somewhat preferable. The factor for cinnamon is entirely independent in heredity of the preceding series of allelomorphs. This factor may be represented by ci and its normal allelo- morph by Ci. The formula for the wild gray would then be Ci Ci, and that for cinnamon would be ci ci. Black would be bb, and the double recessive cinnamon black (or chocolate) would be bb ci ci. Chocolate is one of the commonest types of domesticated mice and since I have used it very extensively in my matings, its relation to the other types may be further stated. It is known that if chocolate is bred to wild gray, and if the gray offspring that are obtained are then inbred, they give, in F,, the following classes: 9 wild gray, 3 cinnamon, 3 black, 1 chocolate. It is clear that chocolate is the double recessive type. Of the two genes, that differentiate chocolate from wild gray, chocolate has one in common with cinnamon and the other with black. In other words, chocolate is cin- namon black, and technically should receive this name. THE EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE Is There a Separate Factor for White-belly? The first series of experiments was made in order to determine whether the peculiarity of white-belly, shown by the wild race of white-bellied grays, is due to a factor that may be separated from the gray white-bellied mice, or whether it is completely linked to gray (or allelo- morphic to it). As wild gray house mice offer some drawbacks in breeding work, I used cinnamon blacks (chocolates). Gray white-bellied mice were bred to 2It is not possible to make a system of allelomorphs (in which the ‘feompounds’’ are serially epistate to each other) consistent entirely with the system of nomenclature that I have suggested for the usual cases in which mutant allelomorphs are contrasted with the normal allelomorphs of the wild (or supposed original) type. 454 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII chocolates.2 The gray white-bellied offspring were se- lected and these were bred again to chocolate. The cross, in regard to sex, was made both ways. If there is an independent factor for white-belly that can separate from the factor for gray gray-belly, then some gray gray- bellied mice should appear. None were obtained, as the following table shows. We must conclude either that there is one factor that gives the gray white-bellied coat, or else that the postulated factor for white-belly is so closely linked to the gray factor that it has not sepa- rated once in 100 times. Therefore unless such a sepa- ration occurs it is simpler to assume one factor for gray white-belly that is allelomorphic to black and to gray gray-belly, ete. TABLE I Gray or Cinna- | | t mon White- Black Chocolate White Mating belly : EE E be | 9 Gwb S by Chao.... 7 21 2 9 5 11 3 1 Ch 92 by Gwb d'.... 2 14 3 10 4 10 Totas. o eaS 9 35 5 19 9 21 3 1 Taking both crosses together, there are 44 grays to 54 blacks and chocolates, which approximate at least to expectation. To these numbers I may add the follow- ing data taken from similar experiments made for other purposes in which one parent was, as before, gray white- belly. Gray-white Belly. Black or Chocolate. 2 g m - 25 20 20 Presumably, therefore, the results may be treated as though a single gene for gray white-belly exists. It will be observed that the experiment has been made in two ways, for at the time I was aware of the possibility that crossing over, if it occured, might be limited to one sex. -3At the time when the experiment was made all the gray white-bellied mice were heterozygous for black and for agouti (including some with the factor for cinnamon). No. 572] MULTIPLE ALLELOMORPHS 455 We are justified, therefore, in treating gray white-belly as an allelomorph of gray gray-belly, the former domi- nating. If crossing over should occur, it might perhaps only be realized in the gray or cinnamon mice, since it is possible that the ticked condition of the hair (that is, common to gray and to cinnamon) is necessary to realize this condition. The expected crossover that would be observed would be gray gray-belly mice. The contrary class would then be black or chocolate mice ‘that carry the factor for white-belly that might or might not show the influence of the supposedly separable factor. My white-bellied stock of mice had been killed after my earlier results had been published, but Mr. B. B. Horton had kept some of my original stock alive, and from him I obtained a few of these mice in 1912 to carry on the above experiments. An extraordinary sex ratio appears in the next to the last table, where there were 26 males to 76 females, ap- proximately 1:3. The mice were entered when about three weeks old. The sex was noted, but no special atten- tion given to the determination. There is some chance of mistaking the sex of young mice, but one familiar with these animals can determine with certainty the sex at three weeks if sufficient care is taken. I have no reason to suppose that I made such errors which would have to be frequent to give these results. If taken, then, at their face value, the data seem to show that there is a sex-linked ‘lethal gene present here. It is not linked to any of the factors involved, and this is not expected, since neither black nor agouti is sex-linked. If further work confirms this conclusion (and I hold it as a provi- sional conclusion until it can be further studied) we have here the first evidence of a sex-linked gene in mice. A sex-linked lethal should give a sex ratio of 14:29. THE ALLELOMORPHISM oF YELLOW, Gray AND BLACK The allelomorphism or ‘‘repulsion’’ of yellow and agouti (gray) may be tested in various ways. One of the 456 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII simplest tests is the following: Yellows were bred to chocolates. The combination gave yellow and agouti off- spring, when certain yellows are used, and yellow and chocolate offspring when other yellows are used. Mixed litters of yellow, agouti and chocolate do not appear. Now when yellow and agouti appear in a given litter (as above) the yellow parent must have carried agouti. If her yellow gene ‘‘repels’’ the agouti gene, then none of the yellow daughters should contain agouti genes, con- sequently if such yellows are next bred to chocolate the offspring should be only yellow and chocolate (or black) and never yellow and agouti. This, in fact, is what my experiments have shown. In the two following tables the results of crossing yellows by chocolates are given by litters. The yellows that were used at first were for the most part heterozygous for gray white-belly, hence in the earlier litters yellows and grays were generally ob- tained. The yellow offspring of these earlier litters were for the most part used in the later experiments, hence the later litters are made up of yellows and chocolates. The records (not given here) showed in every case that yellow mice from litters of yellow and gray gave, when bred to chocolate, only yellows and chocolates. TABLE II YELLOW ¢ BY CHOCOLATE 9? LITTERS | BE Re iin eee sazos assiza zanz ray.. | 5422 Pa: seas ailas oa Chocolate....... ae: alt. lia 2848. 33}. 28/2: 3/428 yen ACEP EER BELICEPELEEPEEECEELEEL LEBEL TABLE III YELLOW 9 BY CHOCOLATE ¢ LITTERS Yellow....4/1/4/4|/3|4|4|2|4|1/4/8/8|1/2/2/s/3/2\/2{1/1 34 Gray..... Pia ue st eve deer of co a, 1 Mt meee Me tio ee pee ee oe eon Chocolate eae Fla] 2/2}../1],.}2/3}4/2/38]}1 Black.. | fefee | | : .|2 White. PEETER RT E i 4 Probably two litters combined, No. 572] MULTIPLE ALLELOMORPHS 457 TABLE IV SUMMARY OF LITTERS Yellow and Gray Yellow and Chocolate T Gray Yel. Choc. 101 78 70 67 The experiment is not demonstrative, however, unless both the yellow daughters and sons are bred to chocolate, for it might be that yellow and agouti are linked and crossing over might occur in one sex and not in the other sex. For instance, if we start again with yellow by choc- olate, then if their yellow offspring contain agouti linked to yellow that does not cross over in one sex, let us say in the males, it follows that a yellow male bred to chocolate would give only yellows and chocolates, for the agouti gene would go with the yellow. Therefore, both sexes must be tested. This essential element in the proof has been overlooked by Little, for he fails to state whether his test experiments were made with both sexes. In my main experiments I have used yellow sons only, and the tables are based on those data, but in a few cases I have mated the yellow daughters (whose brothers were agouti) also to chocolate and have found that these females give only yellows and chocolates, which shows for both sexes that no crossing over of yellow and agouti occurs. A specific case will illustrate this point. A yellow male was bred to a chocolate female and gave 5 yellow and 7 gray offspring in two litters. One of the yellow daughters was bred to chocolate and in four litters pro- duced 11 yellows and 9 chocolates. A yellow grand- daughter gave 9 yellows, 7 chocolates and 4 whites. A yellow female bred to chocolate gave 8 yellows and 16 chocolates, but as I have no record of the preceding generation, I can not be sure that this result is compar- able to the last. It shows at least that a yellow female gave only two kinds of offspring. A ‘‘New Gray’’ Factor A word may be added about the ‘‘new gray.’’ In the original stock obtained from Mr. Horton there was a 458 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII gray female with a not-pure-white belly. She was not used in the main lines of the experiments described above. But she was kept in stock and bred with chocolates. About a year later I noticed in the offspring of a pair of cinnamon white-bellied mice a few mice that looked like chocolates, but which showed, on closer inspection, dis- tinctly ticked hair. One of these new grays bred to black (heterozygous) gave some chocolates, blacks, new grays, and one very dark, almost black, mouse with ticked hair. The female was bred next time to a house mouse (gray gray-belly) and produced all -gray gray- bellied offspring that had a dark coat, but not nearly so dark as that present when the new gray is heterozygous for black. Until further tests have been made it can not be stated whether or not the new factor belongs to the yellow-black system of quadruple allelomorphs. 5 The resemblance of this mouse to the rabbit ‘‘agouti-black’’ homozygous for black is very striking (Punnett, Jour. of Genetics, II, 1912). THIRTEEN YEARS OF WHEAT SELECTION T. B. HUTCHESON ASSOCIATE AGRONOMIST, UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA INTRODUCTION Ty 1901 the Minnesota Agricultural Experiment Sta- tion planted a number of varieties of wheat from the polonicum, spelta, turgidum, durum and vulgare types in foundation beds in order to have specimens of these differ- ent types always on hand for class work, hybridiza- tion or demonstration purposes. Six of these varieties— hedgrow (turgidum), Russian (vulgare), common speltz (spelta), kamouka (durum), and Polish (1) and Polish - (2) (varieties of polonicum)—have been grown continu- ously since that time and an effort has been made to improve them by selection. The method followed was that introduced at this station by Professor W. M. Hays and called the ‘‘centgener’’ method. The centgener method consists, briefly, in starting with individual plants, planting one hundred selected kernels from each plant at equal depths and at equal distances apart in separate plots. A plot of one hundred plants is called a centgener. Careful notes are taken on the plants in each centgener and at harvest time five or more of the highest yielding plants are selected from which the seeds for planting the next year are taken. From these five best plants from five to ten of the best heads are selected and thrashed together. One hundred of the largest and plumpest kernels are then selected out of the seed ob- tained by thrashing these selected heads, and these are planted in the centgener test the next year. This work is continued from year to year, each season the hundred best kernels from the five or more best plants being planted in succeeding centgeners. — 459 460 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII In 1908 an experiment was planned with the object of developing a strain of wheat which would have a minimum amount of culm exposed between the base of the spike and the upper leaf sheath, or in other words, to produce a short-necked variety of wheat. The ultimate purpose of reducing the neck lengths was to reduce the area of the stem exposed to the black stem rust. Since this rust ordinarily does little damage to that portion of the culm enclosed in the leaf sheath, it was thought that a short- necked wheat would be more likely to escape serious damage from stem rust than a long-necked kind. For this work individual plants were selected which had short necks and the seed from these were planted in separate centgeners. Each year at harvest time ten or more plants which appeared to the observer to have the shortest necks were selected from each centgener and measurements of their neck lengths were made and recorded. One hundred kernels were saved from these shortest necked plants each season for subsequent centgeners, thus making a continuous selection for short neck lengths. The data derived from the above experiments seems to throw some light upon the much-discussed question as to whether or not selection within a pure line can increase yield or change type enough to make it a desirable prac- tise from the practical breeder’s standpoint. In both of the experiments, we have the requirements for a pure line satisfied. Wheat is a normally self-fertilized plant. Each centgener was started from a single head in 1901 and these heads have bred true to type ever since. The long period of years over which this experiment has extended makes the data particularly valuable. One of the adverse criticisms to most pure line work is that it has not extended over a long enough period of time. Thirteen years are about as long as any practical breeder would be apt to keep up selection on one pure line and covers the longest period of continuous selection for a self-fertilized plant yet reported. Another criticism to pure line investigations is that in No. 572] THIRTEEN YEARS OF WHEAT SELECTION 461 many cases it has not appeared certain that the material studied was a pure line. Since the plants have bred true to type throughout the whole period of study, it is obvi- ous that this criticism will not hold for the data herein presented. The work has been conducted at this station under the direction of Professor W. M. Hays from 1901 to 1905, under Professor E. C. Parker 1905 to 1908, under Pro- fessor Andrew Boss from 1908 to 1911 and under Pro- -fessor C. P. Bull 1911 to 1913. m3 N = 2. [ N gS IEA | z AN I \ "A SE EE / 2 Atti l a F , eae: | a p LIL X m | A Z VY Puate I. Average yield per plant for all varieties. X-X, fitted straight line. SELECTION To [INCREASE YIELD The varieties studied, the average annual yield of each variety and the average yield per plant for the six vari- eties under test are shown in Table I. In the years 1903 and 1904 weather conditions were unfavorable, making it impracticable to obtain correct average yields per plant, so data for these years were omitted. However, selections of the best plants were made in these two seasons as in the others and the best seed from them were kept for planting, so the continuous selection for increased yield was uninterrupted. 462 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. XLVII TABLE I SHOWING YIELD PER PLANT— YEARS 1901-1913 Yield per Plant in Grs. Name of Variety % j i 1901 | 1902 | 1905 | 1906 | 1907 | 1908 | 1909 | 1910 | 1911 i 1912 | 1913 | | | | FACUITOW . choos oc eee 3.10} 2.80 3.69 2.48) 1.27) 3.75 2.49) 2.55] 2.02) 99 3.67 Riman fos Sea re es 1.00} 1.70} 3.57 1.96) 1. 71) 2.74| 2.71) 2.17 1.95} 1.37} 2.70 BOC. 5 os cece eo ee 2.40} 1.80) 3.99 2.99, 1.38 3.38) 2.40, 2.86, 2.01} 2.14) 2.59 SSHBIOUME ioc ccs a ae ee 1.50} 2.50} 1.99; 2.69) 1. 39 3.31) 2.19} 2.48) 1.67 1.35| 2.16 Po (Be ica. ei a4 ye Bee -80] 1.30) 2.52, 2.04) 1.03) 1.48) 1.91) 1.70) 1. 56, 1.12] 1.74 POMS CA) Dea NEINA, 1.10} .95| 2.83) 1.97 1.26 . $; 61 Lal}. 78 51} 1.33 i | PA T A ET EEA 1.65| 1.84| 3.10 2.35 1.34 2.93 2.22 2.18) 1.83 ioe 2.36 SELECTION TO Increase HEIGHT The average height of the plants for each year of the test is shown in Table II. Though no attempt was made to select for increased height, since a number of workers have shown that height in the small grains is distinctly correlated with yield, it is natural to suppose that the selected plants were among the tallest as well as being the highest yielders of each year’s crop. When this experi- ment was begun, it was not known that height and yield TABLE II SHOWING AVERAGE HEIGHT PER PLANT—1901-1913 Height in Inches Name of Variety T 1901 | 1902 | 1905 | 1906 | 1908 1909 | 1910 | 1911 | 1912 | 1913 Hedgrdee 2... 961-41 | 42 Ea | 46 l 40 | BB t Al 86 1. 86 Roslan... n... 34 | 37 O | 38 44°) 41 | 38 | 36 | 35° | 33 Soltero: S438 Oe | AF 4 at l 80) 40 80: 1 3S Kamouka........ 36 | 34 | 34 | 38 | 40 | 40 | 32 | 38 | 36 | 33 Polish (1)....:...| 40 | 38 | 41 | 38 | 42 |.42 | 33-| 39 | 88./| 83 Polak (0). 6.4 cc 98480 He SP BT ois ss 35 | 84: | 3$ | ar r 3 Average......... 35 | 36 | 38 | 39 | 43 | 40 | 34 | 39 | 36 | 33 were correlated, so the figures on height were kept merely as a matter of general interest and with no idea that they would have bearing on the problem. Among those who later found height correlated with yield are Deneumostier (710),1 Love (711),2 Myers (712),? Leighty (’12)* and 1 Deneumostier, C., ‘‘ Correlations in Wheat,’’ Ann. Gembloux, 20, No. 5, 1910. No. 572] THIRTEEN YEARS OF WHEAT SELECTION 463 SELECTION TO DECREASE Necx-Lenctus The result of the selection for short neck-lengths is shown in Table III. This is a clear illustration of how misleading short-term experiments may be. Had the experiment been discontinued at the end of the third year, the figures would have indicated that it was possible to modify this character very rapidly by selection. How- ever, in the following two years the neck-lengths seemed to revert to the mean of the pure lines, and the last year they were actually longer than when the experiment was started. The reduction in the first three years was prob- ably due to growing conditions. TABLE III SHOWING RESULT OF SELECTION FOR SHORT NECKS Average Neck L ngth in Curve | 1909 1910 1911 1912 | 1918 Dei ed Beever uh Fok 1.86 24 7.34 | 9.54 ite Me ee C 6i 1.12 .79 Gis | 218 bie Ce a | 58 1.65 ‘56 753 | Sete Di | B22 2.08 59 10.47 | 13.82 Discussion From the data presented in these tables, it is evident that there has been no permanent gain for these thirteen years of selection either in yield per plant, height of plant, or shortening of neck-lengths. The expected sea- sonal variations occur. A comparison of the yield of Haynes Blue Stem, which is grown extensively in Minne- sota, and was continued in the variety test without any attempt at selection throughout the whole period, with Hutcheson (713).5 2 Love, H. H., ‘‘A Study of the Large and Small Grain Question,’’ An. Rep. Am. Br. tele: 7: 109-118, 1911 3 Myers, C. H., ‘‘ Variation, Oaren noa and Inheritance of Characters of Wheat and Fea’ Cornell University Thesis, 1912. 4 Hutcheson, T. B., ‘‘Correlated Characters in Avena sativa, with Special Reference to Size of Bd Planted,’’ Cornell University Thesis, 1913. 5 Leighty, C. E., ‘‘Studies in Variation and Correlation of Oats, Avena sativa,’’ Cornell University Thesis, 1912. 464 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVII the average yield of the selected varieties, is shown in Table IV. The average yield in bushels of the Haynes Blue Stem is also platted in comparison with the average yield of the selected varieties in Plate II. In 1912 a severe hail storm injured the variety plats so much that N 2 AN N © Ha N | 2 z ENSEN z = LiNE EAEN / <2. MANIN ee EN / . a EF, NA | } N pi aha ery CNT FA ARE Y7 Kit z = 4% Meaty VY > N | —— ewe mes Prate II. Comparing seasonal fluctuations in selected lines with unse- lected Blue Stem. Solid line, yield per plant in grams for selected lines; dashed line, yield in bushels per acre for Blue Stem it was thought best not to include the yield of the Haynes Blue Stem for that year. This gives an incorrect appear- ance to the curve, as it was extended just as if this year was present and midway between 1911 and 1913 in yield. It will be noticed from Table IV and Plate II that the TABLE IV COMPARING SEASONAL FLUCTUATIONS IN SELECTED LINES WITH UNSELECTED LUE STEM 1901 | 1902 | 1905 | 1906 | 1907 | 1908 | 1909 | 1910 | 1911 | 1912 | 1913 Yield in grs. per plant for selected HEM. cele: 1.65] 1.84) 3.10} 2.35; 1.34] 2.93) 2.22) 2.18) 1.83) 1.24 | 2.36 Yield g ong per acre blue Mem cc eS. 22.9 |23.9 |30.4 |24.00'21.00|26.00/26.6 |24.6 24.2 |..... 23.2 No.572] THIRTEEN YEARS OF WHEAT SELECTION 465 fluctuations from year to year agree very closely. These data indicate that increased yield is due to favorable environmental factors and not to improvement by selection. A comparison of the yield of each variety for the first five years of the test with that of the last five years is shown in Table V. The data in this table show that there is no significant difference in yield for these two periods. In Russian and Polish (1) there is a slight increase in favor of the latter period, but in the other four varieties there is just as much decrease for this period. However, there is not enough difference in any case to indicate either permanent improvement or de- crease in yield. As far as these varieties are concerned, it seems that selection has brought about no permanent improvement. TABLE V COMPARING THE YIELD OF THE Jima YEAR ees WITH THAT OF THE LAST YEAR PERI ist 5-year Period Last 5-year Period Name of Variety Height Yield Height Yield Hedgrow 41.6 2.67 38.4 2.34 38.0 1.99 35.4 2.18 Speltee es 40.0 2.51 39.2 2.40 BAMOURA. o is Vi. s 36.4 2.01 35.8 1.97 Posh soys eae: 39.8 1.54 37.4 1.61 Eola 19) i obras Sue be 33.4 1.62 33.4 131 t AA oa et 38.2 2.06 36.5 1.97 A curve of the yields of the six varieties under con- sideration for the thirteen years of the test was plotted and a straight line was fitted to it, by the method of the least squares, to indicate the trend of the yield. This curve is shown in Plate I. There is a slight downward tendency in this straight line, but it is not enough to indi- cate a tendency toward decrease in yield. The line fitted to the curve of height (Plate III) also shows a slight tendency downward. The data herein cited are not sufficient for definite con- 466 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII clusions. However, the indications are that from a prac- tical breeder’s standpoint permanent improvement in pure lines in small grains, if possible, is certainly not rapid or apt to be very marked. Thirteen years of selec- 4 w4 Nf W z z L \ I a7 + Am = ; ~ 3 Vy l 3 pom -_ = — a Ba Ba = = Puate III. Average height of all varieties. X-X, fitted straight line. tion covers considerable time and expense, and, as far as can be seen from the varieties reported in this paper, it has resulted in no permanent improvement. This would suggest that some other line of improvement must be sought. It is probable that much more rapid progress could be made by segregating pure lines from mixed populations and combining the desirable characters of these lines by hybridization. PATTERN DEVELOPMENT IN MAMMALS AND BIRDS II GLOVER M. ALLEN Boston SOCIETY or NATURAL HISTORY PARTIAL ALBINISM IN Witp MAMMALS Partially albinistie individuals of species that normally are wholly pigmented, occur frequently in a wild state, and almost any large series of a given species may con- tain a few. I have examined many such, in which it was perfectly evident that the white mark was due to areal restriction of some one or more of the primary pigment areas just as described in the various domestic species. It is apparent that the white markings in both are quite comparable, but in species under domestication no agency seems present whereby such pied individuals are elimi- nated, whereas in a wild state the sudden acquisition of a large amount of white in an individual would not only render him too different from his fellows, but might put him at a disadvantage because of a conspicuousness to which as a species he had not yet become accustomed. There are many other species in which, as we now see them, white markings form a permanent and normal part of the pattern. Among those in which these white mark- ings are few or simple, it is often evident that they are merely primary breaks between the pigment patches that have become more or less fixed by long periods of selec- tion, whether natural, sexual or otherwise. As I shall endeavor to show, there are species in which a beginning has already been made towards the development of a pied pattern, though it has not yet become well fixed. Still other species show a more complicated white and pigmented pattern, the white portions of which can not readily be derived from primary breaks alone. Such I take to be highly developed patterns and make no attempt 467 468 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII to analyze them here. Examples of this type are seen in the zebra, the spotted skunks (Spilogale), the striped weasel (Ictonyx). Probably more than one factor is responsible for some of the combinations of stripes and spots seen, for example, in certain spermophiles (Citellus 13-lineatus), but I shall not now attempt a discussion of these. One of the most frequent manifestations of pigment reduction in mammals is the presence of a white spot in the normally pigmented forehead. This is due primarily to the reduction of the ear patches, which fail to meet at their median edges. Perhaps, too, the apparent loss of the crown patch in some mammals still further tends to lessen the amount of pigment production at this point. Rabbits and hares very often have more or less white in the forehead, but none of the species has developed this sufficiently to make it a permanent mark. Moseley in his . ‘“‘ Naturalist on the Challenger,” speaks of a ‘‘black variety” of wild rabbit—doubtless introduced—‘‘with a white spot on the forehead’’ as occasionally found on Teneriffe, Canary Islands, but this mark is common, and I have seen it in such widely sundered species as the eastern varying hare of New Hampshire and the black- necked hare native to Java. A specimen of Leisler’s bat (Nyctalus leisleri) in the Museum of Comparative Zool- ogy has a white spot in the middle of the forehead and another on the mid-ventral line of the abdomen—the first a primary break between the ear centers, the second probably a ventral primary break between those of the sides. Among the Insectivora, the West Indian Solen- odon paradoxus has a white patch at the nape of the neck which has become a permanent part of its pattern. It is clearly the enlargement of a primary break sepa- rating the ear patches and neck patches on the median dorsal line. It is a fact of much interest that in a con- = siderable series of this species in the collection of the Museum of Comparative Zoology hardly two have it developed alike, but it varies from a few white hairs to No. 572] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 469 a large patch 15 X 10 mm. wide. Evidently it has not yet become precisely defined in its limits, though now a permanent mark of the species. White marks in the forehead are common among the species of the Mustelide or weasel family. A narrow white median line is present in the Javan mydaus and in the skunks (Mephitis) as part of the permanent pattern. In the badger (Taxidea) a white line is not only pres- ent on the forehead, but it is often extended medially so as to separate the pigment patches of both sides of the body. In the New York weasel (Mustela noveboracensts) of the eastern United States a few white hairs are often present on the forehead, and other instances could be multiplied. Among monkeys, a white spot on the nose is present in some species of Lasiopyga, and in an allied genus Rhinostigma, it is elongated vertically to form a white streak. A yet more illuminating case is that of the Muskeget Beach mouse (Microtus breweri) a derivative of the common brown meadow mouse of the New England mainland. On this island of white sand off the Massa- chusetts coast, a pale variety has developed which is very distinct from that of the neighboring shores. Not only is it a paler race, but albinism also has begun to appear, so that occasional individuals have a white fleck between ° the ears, showing the drawing apart of the ear patches. Of a series of 62 specimens in the collections of the Museum of Comparative Zoology and the Boston Society of Natural History, no less than 13 had such white flecks, and one had in addition a white spot just in advance of the shoulders, marking the line of separation between neck and shoulder patches. In our studies on the hered- ity of coat colors in mice, Professor Castle and I dis- covered (Allen, 1904; see also Little, 1914) that the pied condition is recessive in the Mendelian sense towards the self colored, so that partial albinos bred to wholly pigmented mice produce in the second generation, if interbred, 25. per cent. of spotted young. The figures 470 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII given above (13 in 62) are near this in case of the Muske- get mouse, but the matings are of course more promiscu- ous. The case is interesting in connection with the studies of Ramaley (1912) and Pearl (1914), tending to show that in a mixed population the recessives may in- crease so as to exceed the dominants. Although the spotted mice do not, in case of this species, exceed the unspotted individuals, they nevertheless are of far more frequent occurrence than they are in the mainland repre- sentatives of the species. This accords with the fact that island-living mammals are very commonly albinistic, and the cause is doubtless that the population is much more inbred, so that the recessive condition of partial albinism is more likely to be propagated than if successive genera- tions have a wider range over which to spread. It seems probable that heredity will tend to increase the propor- tion of spotted mice of Muskeget, and that if this condi- tion is disadvantageous, a large part of the spotted indi- viduals will be killed off, yet in the course of time they may become adjusted to this condition and will survive in increasing proportion till the white mark becomes characteristic of all the animals. Cory (1912) records the capture of seven muskrats at Hayfield, Iowa, all of which were uniformly marked, having a white ring around the neck and the entire underparts, feet, and end of tail white. I can think of three causes influencing the status of such white markings. These markings may be in- herited in a purely automatic way as unit characters; but if thus inherited they may be (1) increased through selection, natural or sexual; or (2) eliminated by the same agent; or (3) they may be, at first, of’no influence at all in the economy of the animal and persist or not, accord- ing as they are heritable. I have mentioned that island mammals tend to be more albinistic than their mainland representatives. Other cases may be mentioned, as the common squirrel (Sciurus vulgaris leucurus) of Great Britain, which differs nota- bly from that of the continent in having frequently a No. 572] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 471 white or whitish tip to the tail, often for one half its length. A similar white tip is occasionally seen in our red ` squirrel (S. hudsonius) as an albinistic mark, and is due, of course, to the terminal restriction of the rump patches. The deer of Whitby Island, Puget Sound, are said to be much marked with white, and sundry marsupials of Papua as well as the monotreme Zaglossus are subject to white markings. In the cuscus (Pseudochirus) the pig- ment is sometimes restricted to small patches and round spots scattered on the back, those in the region of the shoulder of a different color from those of the side and rump patches. Another instance is that of the white- footed mouse of Monomoy Island, Massachusetts, the mid-ventral parts of which are pure white to the roots of the hairs, an albinistic condition to be clearly distin- guished from that in which the belly appears white, but only because of the white tips to the hairs whose bases are dark-pigmented. The restriction of the rump patches so as to produce a white tail-tip is common among mammals. It is found in occasional specimens of many species as the shrew mole (Blarina), Brewer’s mole (Parascalops), the meadow jumping mouse (Zapus), the white-footed mouse (Pero- myscus), and squirrels (Sciurus). In some it has be- come developed as a permanent and characteristic mark, as in the woodland jumping mouse (Napeozapus), the red fox (Vulpes), such genera as Hydromys, Tylomys, the Virginia opossum (Didelphys virginiana), the tree kangaroos (Dendrolagus). In many others a pure white belly is developed through ventral restriction of the shoulder and side patches. Among ungulates the break between the ear patches has been developed to form a broad white blaze from forehead to nose in case of the blesbok (Damaliscus albifrons) of South Africa and in related species in East Africa. The chevron-mark on the forehead of cer- tain antelopes is possibly a specialized development of the same thing. 472 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII White buttock patches are present in several unrelated ungulates—as the pronghorn (Antilocapra), the wapiti (Cervus canadensis), and the Rocky Mountain sheep: (Ovis canadensis). Probably these are the result of restriction or total inactivity of the pigment patches covering the rump. Fic. 42a. DIAGRAM SHOWING THE PIGMENTED PATCHES OF A PARTIALLY ALBINO Among the deer family white is generally confined to the under surfaces and the primary white breaks have not been developed to form patterns. Albinistic deer are fairly common, however, and in Fig. 42a I have made a tracing from a photograph showing the side of a par- tially albino doe in which areal restriction of pigment has taken place in such wise that the primary patches are all indicated, and separated from those of the opposite half of the body by a median dorsal white line. The ear and the neck patches are joined, but a few small islands of pigment are left here and there, much as in cows. In the young of many deer and in the adult of such species as the axis deer, a spotted pattern is developed. No. 572] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 473 There is an obvious tendency for the spots to become arranged in longitudinal rows, and intermediate stages may be found in which they coalesce to form broken lines, There is little doubt that the complete white stripes occurring in part of this pattern were formed originally through the coalescence of rows of white spots. In the tapir a somewhat similar spotted pattern is found in the young, while the adult Malayan tapir has lost the shoulder and side patches, producing thus a white-bodied animal, pigmented to the back of the foreleg and on the buttocks and hind legs. Among the ground squirrels (Citellus) a beautiful series can be picked out showing the transition from a uniform grizzled mixture of ticked hairs to indistinct spotting, then rows of white spots, and finally broken and complete longitudinal stripes. The production of these stripes I believe to be due, not to the development of breaks between the primary pigment patches, but to the action of a factor which is the negative of the so-called ‘‘ English’? marking in rabbits, so that instead of the development of scattered small pigments spots there are formed, instead, spots without pigment. That it is possible to evolve a striped pattern from spots through selection, I have no doubt, and indeed, it is gen- erally believed. On the other hand, it is quite possible that the converse may happen, and spots result through the breaking up of stripes. According to the experiments of Professor Castle and Dr. MacCurdy, however, it seems to be a difficult matter to fix a given marking by rigid selection, yet it must be admitted that a few years’ work even of careful breeding is nothing in comparison with the age-long selection that may have been at work on the species: That it is a difficult matter to produce a given pattern is further evidenced by the fact that in many species in which white markings regularly occur as part of the pattern, these are subject to great individual variation in their extent, showing that sie are even yet not wholly definite. It was formerly urged against a dpetvine 474 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII that we do not now see its processes in action, that species are stable and subject to very little variation. This view, however, was found to rest on faulty observation, for, though some species are fairly stable, others are very plastic and exhibit before our eyes various steps in development. So in case of the development of a partic- ular pied pattern, it is possible to see in certain species the actual course of its formation. Among mammals, the Mustelide or weasel family show several instances in is A Oe an 7 4$ Fics. 43-48. DIAGRAMS SHOWING RESTRICTION OF PIGMENTATION ON THE VEN- TRAL SURFACE OF MINKS (Mustela Aani, point. The common mink (Mustela vison) of north- eastern North America is now in process of developing a pure white under side, such as is present in the New York weasel (M. noveboracensis) or the smaller Bonaparte’s weasel (M. cicognani). The diagrams shown in Figs. 43-48 are from the fine series of mink in the collection of the Museum of Comparative Zoology and depicit the No. 572] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 475 under side of the specimens. In the large coastal race of mink found from southern Maine to the Carolinas (M. v. lutreocephalus), the entire pelage is usually brown, ex- cept for the chin which is white. Occasional white marks are present in some specimens along the mid-ventral line of the throat and chest, and between the hind legs. In the smaller typical M. vison of northern New England northward the white marking is apt to be more extensive, and in no two individuals exactly alike. The diagrams show the ventral markings of a few specimens from New England and Nova Scotia. In Fig. 43 the amount of white is very small. The chin spot, which represents the beginning of a break between the two ear patches at their antero-ventral extremity, is always present and has be- come now a fixed mark of the species, though variable in extent. A slight break in the center of the chest shows where the two shoulder patches have failed to meet, and a white spot at the anal region indicates a like restriction of the rump patches. Similar spots appear mid-ventrally in Fig. 44, with the addition of a few white hairs, medially at the upper throat, where the ear and neck patches join, and a few more on the lower throat at the line of union of the neck patches of opposite sides. In Figs. 45 and 46 no break is present on the abdomen, but in the former figure, a large transverse break has appeared on the upper throat where the ear patches fail to unite with the neck patches and with each other, and a median line runs forward to join the white of the chin, showing the greater restriction of the ear patches ventrally. An imperfect separation of these patches along the center of the throat has taken place in Fig. 47, and a more considerable break occurs in the same place in Fig. 46. In the Pacific Coast mink (Mustela vison energumenos) a well-developed white patch on the chest is rather characteristic, some- what larger than in Fig. 45. This is due to the ventral restriction of the shoulder patches which fail to meet below. In Fig. 46 this white area is seen with a tongue extending upon the center of the lower throat, and on to 476 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII one fore leg, as well as in the mid line of the thorax, mark- ing nearly the anteroposterior limits of the shoulder patch. The neck patches are not separated in this figure but have become so in Fig. 48, so that a continuous line of white runs from chin to chest. In Fig. 47 the shoulder and the side patches have both failed to join ventrally, and thus a broad white line is formed down the center of the: belly from the conjoined neck patches to the rump patches. If all these breaks were to be present in a single animal, there would be a narrowed white area along the entire ventral side of the body from chin to anus, extend- ing on to the lower side of the fore legs. Practically this condition exists in another species of the same genus, Streator’s weasel (Mustela streatori) of the Pacific Coast, in which the throat, chest and belly are white but the width and boundaries of the white area are very variable in different individuals. It is therefore in a stage beyond that which the minks have reached, yet it has not attained the stage in which the white area is of definite and rather constant bounds, as in certain other weasels, for example Mustela noveboracensis, in which the white, of the belly extends nearly or quite to the lateral border of the body, but in different individuals varies slightly, and M. cicognanii, in which the white area of the belly constantly extends to the lateral boundary of the venter from throat to anus. This is the condition toward which the mink is tending. Another interesting case in which a pattern mark ap- pears to be evolving through the fixation of a primary break between pigment patches is that of the so-called tayra of South America (Tayra barbara) a large Muste- lid. The Central American race (biologie) of this animal is wholly black, but the typical subspecies of Brazil and northern South America is subject to a varying amount of reduction in pigmentation. Curiously, this takes place at the posterior end of the neck patches or at the anterior part of the shoulder patches. Three of five specimens in the Museum of Comparative Zoology are marked in this No. 572] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 477 way. All have a triangular patch of white at the base of the throat ventrally, as a break between neck and shoulder patches and a partial separation of the neck patches from each other. Each has a dorsal mark of white; in the first a narrow linear break between the shoulders; in the second a broader transverse mark, and in the third a square patch of white occupying nearly the width of body between the shoulders to the base of the neck. The white throat marking increases in extent from first to third, just as does the dorsal marking. Probably in time this white mark, now of irregular size and appearance indi- vidually, will become a permanent part of the pattern. In this animal the entire head and neck are a grizzled gray as far back as the posterior limit of the neck patches, and the rest of the body is black. This, then, shows that the pigment patches of head and neck are differentiated in color as well, from the patches of the rest of the body. The occurrence of white markings in the back is relatively uncommon in mammals, though white on the under sur- faces is common, and, as shown by Mr. Abbott H. Thayer, may be of real service to the animal as a factor in con- cealment. In the development of white pattern-marks, the evi- dence seems to show that these come in at first as small and fluctuating spots, which may be of little effect in the economy of the animal. Their further development might lead to the extinction of the species if they render it too conspicuous to enemies, unless the species at the same time makes use of them or accommodates itself to their re- vealing effect. Often, no doubt, they may not be a source of danger at all. A case in point may be that of Sciurus finlaysoni, a Malayan squirrel, most of the individuals of which are largely marked with white, and of which speci- mens may be found side by side, varying from an almost entirely pigmented condition to one of completely white coat and black eyes. White squirrels are occasional in other species, as albinos, but these rarely survive more than a generation in the cases I have known, whereas 478 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII Finlayson’s squirrel seems to have accustomed itself by gradual stages to the white condition, so that it is prob- ably not at a great disadvantage by reason of its whiteness. Piement Patcues In Bms In birds the same primary pigment patches seem to be present as in mammals, and they are homologous in the two groups. In defining the extent of the pigment patches, however, allowance must be made for the fact that the long feathers may cover a part of the body remote from their origin. The distribution of the feathers or the pterylosis of the species in hand must also be r bered. In order to arrive at the true interpretation of the patches, it is necessary to consider the pigment as projected back from the vanes of the feathers to the part of the body at their bases. By so doing, it becomes evident that a feather variegated with pigmented and unpigmented (or white) areas indicates none the less that the feather arises from a place of pigment formation. It is only a wholly white feather or patch of feathers that can be considered albinistic in the sense here intended. The factor determining the intermittent formation of pig- ment in the individual feather is probably a wholly different one from that determining the presence or ab- sence of pigment formation at certain places on the body, though not necessarily different except in its inter- mittent action. In the domestic pigeon of our streets and buildings, we have a species that in its wild state is normally fully pigmented except for a white rump patch. Under semi- domestication it has developed partial albinism to a large degree, so that it is possible to obtain a complete series representing on the one extreme a totally pigmented bird without a trace even of the white rump patch, and on the other extreme a bird of pure white plumage. A few of the intermediate stages in areal reduction of pig- mentation are shown in Figs. 49 to 53, selected from birds raised for the market and, so far as known, not No. 572] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 479 bred for pattern. The first steps in reduction are shown in Fig. 49. Here there is seen first a crescentic band of white feathers passing from eye to eye around the occi- put. This is a primary break marking off the crown patch posteriorly. This patch in birds, in contrast to its development in mammals, is the main patch of the head, 4g so oF Fics. 49-53. DIAGRAMS SHOWING PIGMENTATION IN THE DOMESTIC PIGEON. covering the area from the base of the bill to the eyes and occiput. In Fig. 50 its posterior limit is similarly defined by a primary break separating it from the neck patches, and although it does not extend forward quite to the eye in this specimen, it shows a beginning of sepa- ration from the more lateral ear patches by virtue of the indentations on each side posteriorly. In Fig. 52, the crown patch is shown slightly reduced in extent and wholly separate from the ear patches, which have become inactive altogether. In Fig. 53 it has dropped out with the latter. It is evident then that by greater or lesser 480. THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII reduction of the crown patch alone it is possible to pro- duce a pigeon with a mere white spot at the back of the head, one with a white stripe from the base of the beak through the eyes to the back of the head (or some part of such a stripe) to a pigeon in which by the total reduc- tion of the patch, the entire top of the head is white. Such specimens can be found in most any miscellaneous flock. There is a tendency often for the patch to be irreg- ularly broken, sometimes divided almost into two parts, a result of the pterylosis to some extent. The ear patches in pigeons, and probably in all birds, are rather insignificant, and the smallest of all the pri- mary pigment areas. “They include the feathers from the posterior angle of the lower mandible to the angle of mouth and thence back, including the ear coverts. I do not feel sure that the patches of opposite sides may not join on the chin, but the present evidence tends to show that the chin is pigmented by a forward extension of the neck, patch, which, under reduction, often leaves a small island of pigment between the mandibular rami. In Fig. 50 the neck patches are seen to have broken away ante- riorly from the crown and ear patches and the separa- tion of the latter from the crown is indicated by deep reentrants along the line of the separation. In Fig. 51 a remnant of the ear patch of the left side alone remains in dorsal view, consisting of a small tuft of pigmented feathers at the fore end of the aural area and a single pigmented feather just behind it. In this specimen there are a few pigmented feathers on the chin as well, which I take to be an isolated bit of the neck patches. The neck patches are bilateral in origin, and pigment the entire throat and neck back to a point corresponding to the base of the neck vertebre. They meet the crown patch and separate the ear patches at the occiput. In the domestic pigeon the neck patches correspond very closely to the area of differentiated feathers that give the metal- lic reflections. In the reduction of this area it is common for the anterior part of the throat to be white, and then a No. 572] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 481 break occurs between the neck patches and those of the head as in Fig. 50. Posteriorly the neck patch under reduction may become separated by a white ring at the base of the neck, from the shoulder patches as in Fig. 50. The ultimate centers of these patches seem to be in the pigeon well back on the base of the neck. These are shown, of small extent, in Fig. 51, as two small areas of pigmented feathers, one on each side of the base of the neck. In Fig. 53, further reduction has taken place, so that the patch of the left side only remains as a small center. In Fig. 52 there is a large median dorsal patch, which, as in mammals, may represent the two centers of opposite sides which even under much reduction have not in this individual become divided medially. A very common manifestation of pigment reduction in pigeons is to have the primaries or some of them white, as in Figs. 49 or 50., This indicates a failure of pigment to develop at the extremities of the shoulder patches, just as in mammals white forefeet mark a slight reduc- tion of the same areas. It is a fact of much interest that in the guinea fowl (Numida), which has been under domestication but a short time comparatively, a distinct breed has arisen in which this same reduction of pigment is present, resulting in a speckled bird with pure white primaries and often a pure white area on the breast. In the pigeon, further reduction cuts off a narrow ring of pigment encircling the breast, or, it may be, broken in the mid-ventral line. This ring represents the reduced shoulder patches, and is to be seen in many wild species as a permanent part of the pattern. The white collar at the base of the neck in Fig. 50 marks the separation between the neck and the shoulder patches at the ante- rior border of the latter. In other specimens the patches are separated medially by a white area down the back. The ultimate centers of these patches seem to be near the elbow or on the upper arm at the base of the tertiaries, as seen in Figs. 52 and 53. 482 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII The side patches are rather small and seem to center, as in Fig. 51, near the groin on either side. They pig- ment the belly back of the breast area included by the shoulder patches, and extend on to the hind legs as well. In a specimen before me, the shoulder patches pigment the bases of the wings and the entire breast correspond- ing roughly to the length of the sternum, and tend to be separated by encroaching white feathers midventrally. The side patches are much more reduced, and are con- fined to a small area at the top of each thigh. The re- mainder of the patches has become inactive, so that a completely white belly and back result. A very common occurrence is the white rump patch due to the restriction of the side patches, so that a break occurs between them and the tail patches. The rump patches in birds are situated far back, as in mammals, and pigment the tail coverts and the rectrices as in Figs. 49-51. The bilater- ality of the two patches is often indicated in pigeons by the occurrence of a few pure white rectrices in the center of the tail. Other birds show pure white feathers at either side of the tail, with a tendency to bilateral sym- metry, a most important fact, since it indicates restriction at the outer extremes of these centers. In the restriction of pigment formation, the rectrices are the first to be- come white, as one would expect, since they are situated at the extremity of the body and farthest from the center of the patch. In Fig. 52 these centers are seen to be at the base of the tail above, and include the upper tail coverts. They are still joined medially, but that of the left side is more extensive than the patch on the right side. The approximate boundaries of the several pigment patches are indicated in Fig. 53 by dotted lines; 1 is the crown patch, 2 the ear patch, 3 the neck patch, 4 the shoulder patch, 5 the side, and 6 the rump patch, as they appear in a dorsal view. Ventrally the neck patch runs forward to the symphysis of the mandibles. In a flock of domesticated mallard ducks which I studied, the same patches were found indicated, and No. 572] ‘PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 483 some of the details of these are shown in Figs. 54-56. In the male wild mallard there is no white in the pattern of the head and neck except a white ring at the base of the neck. In one of the domesticated breed, shown in Fig. 54, the crown patch was very beautifully marked off, as in the pigeon (Fig. 49), by a white band from eye to eye passing about the occiput. This duck was further interesting in showing the median division of the two neck patches, as a narrow white line running down the <7 Jsf 5e Fé Fies, 54-57. DIAGRAMS SHOWING PIGMENTATION IN DOMESTICATED MALLARD DUCKS AND IN THE (WILD) LABRADOR DUCK (57). back of the neck medially, from the occipital stripe. An- other duck shown in Fig. 55 had lost the neck patches entirely, but showed the same occipital stripe bounding the crown patch posteriorly, and the ear patches dorsally. The ear patches still adjoin the crown patch anteriorly. In Fig. 56 is represented another of these ducks in which both ear patches are distinct and separate on either side of the head. The crown patch appears as two narrow lines of pigmented feathers which are not quite in con- tact posteriorly. I have not obtained a satisfactory explanation for the apparent tendency of this patch to 484 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII divide medially. Probably for some reason the forma- tion of the pigment is more intense at the sides of the crown than in the center where the nerve and blood supply is less. In the pterylosis of this area the develop- ment of feathers is seen to be greater at the sides also. The neck patches and the side patches are absent en- tirely, but the shoulder patches are both present, in Fig. 56, that of the right side covering the scapulars and middle of the upper back, that of the left side including a few only of the scapulars. The tail patches are both present, and separate from each other, as shown by the median white rectrices. In this same flock of mallards was a female which had a white ring at the base of*the neck in the same situation as the white ring which in the male is a part of the per- manent pattern. It was not quite complete dorsally, however, in this female, and was somewhat broader than regularly in the male. Nevertheless, it is apparent that this white collar in the male is merely a primary break between neck and shoulder patches that has become developed as a part of the normal pattern. - Stone (1912, p. 318) in his paper on the phylogenetic value of color characters in birds, hints at the existence of these patches. He says, in part: In matters of pattern there seems to be a deeper problem involved, i. e., the determination of the cause governing the appearance of a dif- ferently colored patch on corresponding parts of the plumage of birds belonging to wholly different groups . . . or the presence of a mystacial stripe, a superciliary stripe, a light rump patch. .. . In fact if a bird exhibits a bright or contrasting patch of color, it is, in the vast major- ity of cases, found on one of several definite portions of the plumage, as the crown, the throat, the bend of the wing, the rump, ete. These contrasting areas are due to the development of one or more of the primary patches, or of breaks between them, or again paler areas, as at the bend of the wing or on the rump, indicate often a lessening of pigment inten- sity at a distance from the respective primary centers. (To be concluded.) NOTES ON THE MEADOW JUMPING MOUSE (ZAPUS HUDSONIUS) ESPECIALLY RE- GARDING HIBERNATION H. L. BABCOCK, M.D. DEDHAM, Mass. THE jumping mouse is the only one of the wild mice of this region (Massachusetts) which exhibits the habit of regular hibernation. Regarding this habit there are a number of references in the literature on the subject. Barton! was one of the first to refer to the fact that this mouse became dormant in winter. He says, in describing the actions of one he had in captivity: On or about the 22d of November it passed into the torpid state. It is curious to observe that at the time it became torpid the weather was unusually mild for the season of the year, and moreover the animal was kept in a warm room, in which there was a large fire the greater part of the day and night . It was frequently most active while the weather was extremely ld in December. This was in Philadelphia, Pa. : Audubon and Bachman? regret that they live in a region where the species does not exist and can not speak from personal observation on the subject. Godman,: Thompson‘ and Kennicott® speak of its habit of hrboriation: Tenney® gives an account of a specimen of this species taken alive on January 18, 1872, near Vincennes, Ind. It was dormant, coiled up tightly, ‘‘the nose being placed upon the belly, and the long tail coiled around the ball-like 1‘*Some Account of an American Species of Dipus or Jerboa,’’ by Ben- jamin Smith Barton, M.D., Translations of the Am. Philosophical Society, Vol. IV, No. XII, 1799. 2 Viviparous Quadrupeds of No. Anais: ’ Vol. II, 1851, p. 255. 8 Godman, ‘‘Am, Nat. Hist.,’’ Vol. I, 1 4 Rev. Zadoe Thompson, Ni at. and Civil Hk of Vermont,’’ 1842. 5 Kennicott, Patent Office Report for 1857. 6 Tenney, ‘‘ Hibernation of the Jumping Mouse,’’ AM. NATURALIST, June, 1872, Vol. VI, No. 6, pp. 330-332. 485 486 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII form which the animal had assumed.’’ It was taken from a nest about two feet below the surface, made of bits of grass. The mouse showed no signs of life at first, but on being held in his hand, soon became feebly active, and on being placed in a warm room, came out of its dormant con- dition entirely. It again became dormant that night, but was aroused twice again by the application of heat, within the next few weeks, in spite of very cold weather. Merriam’ tells of taking an active male at Easthamp- ton, Mass., on February 11, 1872, and states that during the mild winter of 1881-82, in Lewis County, Northern New York, he saw jumping mice active several times. Seton® speaks of finding a Zapus Hudsonius on Sep- tember 27, 1888, at Carberry, Manitoba, in a nest of leaves under the roots of a stump, nearly torpid. He says: In the country near Carberry, I never saw it active after September t: Stone and Cram? believe that this mouse passes six months or more of every year hibernating underground. They speak of seeing a family of them turned up by a plough in May and exhibiting not the slightest symptom of life, on being handled or breathed upon. Burroughs!’ tells of a female jumping mouse in cap- tivity that began hibernating early in November and con- tinued until May, with several intervals of activity, espe- cially after warm weather came on. Preble! says: Hibernation varies with the locality, but usually begins about the time of the first heavy frosts and lasts until Spring. The fall pelage is usually assumed and the animals become exceedingly fat before entering winter quarters. Although they often lay up stores of food in nests or urrows during summer, it is not known that they use this food during winter. The animals are generally found singly (sometimes in pairs) in nests at a depth varying from a few inches to two or three feet below the surface. Hibernation sometimes takes place above ground. TC. H. Merrian, M.D., ‘‘Mammals of the Adirondack ai 7? 1884. 8 E. T. Seton, “Life histories of Northern Animals,’’ Vol. I. 9 Stone and Cram, ‘‘ American Animals,’’ p. 103- 104. 10 John Burroughs, ‘‘ Squirrels and Other Fur Bearers,’’ pp. 121-12 11 E, A. Preble, ‘‘ Revision of the Pa Mice of the Genus ue U. S. Dept. Agr. N. A. Fauna Series, No. 15, 1899. No.572] NOTES ON MEADOW JUMPING MOUSE 487 On June 25, 1912, a female Zapus Hudsonius was taken alive, by the writer, on the edge of a small pond in eastern Massachusetts. It was placed in a small wire cage, and after a few frenzied efforts to escape, became quite tame. On July 5 it gave birth to five young, blind and hairless; but when the family was transferred to a larger cage, the mouse deserted the young and they soon died. One dis- appeared mysteriously, and may have been eaten by the mother. The young measured at birth: total length 33 mm.; tail 9 mm.; hind foot 4 mm. Throughout the summer the mouse ate chiefly rolled oats and shredded wheat, and was also very fond of straw- berries and blueberries. It refused most of the common fruits and vegetables. It was almost wholly nocturnal in its activity, sithough when disturbed during the day it would immediately begin to eat and remain active for half an hour or more. Toward the latter part of the summer, it seemed to grow quite fat. Rhoads!” says in this connection: When going into winter quarters they are exceedingly fat, as I can testify from experience in removing this tenacious yellow blanket from the skins of them. This fat is their fuel. By spring it is nearly gone. During the latter part of August there were several very cool nights (49° F. minimum) and on the night of August 28 it did not come out. This fact was apparent from the clean drinking dish, which was placed in such a position that the mouse could not approach without scat- tering saw-dust in it. The absence was repeated on Au- gust 30, and September 1. Throughout September its actions were irregular. Every night until the 21st, with the exception of the 12th and 17th, it was active, but on the 22d disappeared for four nights. It was then active for two more nights (26 and 27) and following that, inactive for six (September 28 to October 3). From Oc- tober 4 to 28 it was out every night, although not as vigor- ous as formerly, neither did it eat as much. When ap- proached it seemed to pay no heed, as if in a sort of stupor. 12 S. N. Rhoads, ‘The Mammals of Pennsylvania and New Jersey.’’ 488 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII OrriciA. OBSERVATIONS From US. Wearner Bureau, Gos row Sranion. CHART vlad MINIMUM. NIGHTLY Aveust SEPTEM Aaa 12 1431F 1516 IT IF 19-10 H 222324 3S 3 2) 38 3430 Hf 23¢F6 759, Utd 7 70.2 y 729 293P 72347 70" bs éo’ 58 ts? vo" 3s 30% as © = NIGHTS ON WHICH THE MOUSE WAS NOT ACTIVE, There was no evidence of any attempt at storing away a supply of food, although there was ample opportunity. This habit of storing food is mentioned by Hornaday** who says: In the autumn it stores in the ground quantities of food for winter use, but despite this fact, under certain conditions, it becomes so Bas oughly dormant in winter that it seems to be quite acinar According to Seton,® ` It is quite ready to respond at any time to any spell of unusually fine, unseasonable weather, even in the depths of winter, and it is probably for these arousing times, as much as for the spring time famine, that it lays up its abundant stores of food. Preble"! also mentions this habit, but Shufeldt'* denies it. He says, in speaking of the deer mouse (Peromyscus Leucopus) : Is it to meet the requirements of his condition that this mouse lays up a goodly stock of food during the autumn? one the Zapus does not do. Following the period of activity through October, the mouse was inactive on the four nights of October 28, 29, 30 and 31, and reappeared for the last time on the night of November 1, after which it retired for the winter. The cage was placed by an open window of an empty box stall in a stable where the temperature was practically that of out doors. The mouse built its nest in the side of a large sod placed in one corner of the cage. In spite of a very mild winter, the lowest official tem- 13 Hornaday, ‘‘The American Natural History.’’ 14R. W. Shufeldt, M.D., ‘‘Chapters on the Natural History of the United States.’’ No. 572] NOTES ON MEADOW JUMPING MOUSE 489 TEMPERATURES, AVGUST — NOVEMGER, 1912, OCTOBER ; NOVEMBER STII 1M RISHSKIZI GG Wr PALAGIAN 23 ESE JE FION IMIS 17M IPOH > ia AWN BY f H.L. BABCOCK: perature for this section being only 3° F. (February 10, 1913,) the mouse did not survive the cold weather, and was found dead, when the cage was opened on June 17, 1913. The nest was found to be located in the extreme end of the sod, only 14 inches from the top and about 1 inch from the edge. It was roughly oval in shape, being hollowed out of the loam and lined with a few blades of grass. It _ measured roughly 13 inches by 14 inches and was just large enough to contain the mouse when curled up into a ball. The opening was on the side. Death was probably caused from exposure to continued cold owing to the un- protected location of the nest. The poor judgment shown in not building the nest securely in the middle of the large sod, and other similar instances of poor management, have led the writer to believe that the intelligence (if that term may be used) of the Zapus Hudsonius is of comparatively low grade, much lower, for instance, than that of the deer mouse (Peromys- cus Leucopus). The accompanying chart, which is a record of minimal nightly temperatures, according to the official observa- tions of the U. S. Weather Bureau for this section (Bos- ton), shows the activity of the mouse in relation to the temperature during August, September, October and No- vember, and brings out some rather interesting facts. For example, on October 15, 16 and 17, with the minimum nightly temperature 42°, 36° and 42°, respectively, the mouse was active, while on September 22, 23, 24 and 25, 490 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII with the minimum nightly temperature of 48°, 49°, 56° and 53° F., respectively, a much warmer series of nights, it remained inactive, And again, after November 1, dur- ing a warm spell in which the minimum nightly tempera- ture for November 7 was 64° F., the mouse did not appear. A study of this chart suggests the question as to how much the temperature has to do with this habit of hiber- nation. It is a somewhat general belief that temperature regu- lates the degree of torpidity. Barton! maintains that the torpid state of animals is altogether an accidental circumstance and by no means constitutes a specific character. The same species becomes torpid in one country and not in another. Nay, different individuals of the same species become torpid or continue awake in the same neigh- borhood or even on the same farm. Seton® believes that while torpor is more or less controlled by temperature, the habit of tor- pidity, like the changing pelage of the white-hare, is so deeply ingrained constitutionally that there is a strong tendency to torpify at a given time without regard to the original cause. It is evident from this chart that torpidity develops gradually, at first for only one night at a time. Whether this process is explained by a cerebral anemia, a slow toxemia of the brain centers or some other of the theories regarding sleep, it seems to require about two months in which to become sufficiently developed to control com- pletely voluntary body functions. During that interval the animal occasionally awakens, probably from hunger and habit as much as from any effect in change of tem- perature. After torpidity is thoroughly established, changes of temperature may be important external factors, as has been demonstrated on numerous occasions in producing a temporary activity by the application of heat. It is safe to say, however, that the temperatire is not the only ele- ment which influences the length of the period of hiberna- tion. SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION STUDIES ON INBREEDING—IV ~ ON A GENERAL FORMULA FOR THE CONSTITUTION OF THE NTH GENERATION OF A MENDELIAN POPULATION IN WHICH ALL MATINGS ARE OF BROTHER X SISTER ?* _ I. In a former paper in this series’ the constitution of a Men- delian population in which all mating was of the brother X sister type was worked out empirically. The results there pre- sented may be put in the form of a general formula, by means of which the constitution of any generation may be written down from a knowledge of the preceding generation; that is from a knowledge of the n— 1th generation the nth generation may be at once written down. II. This general formula may be developed as follows. A single character pair will be considered, A denoting the dominant character and a the recessive. Equal fertility for all matings is assumed, the number of individuals per family being taken as 2s, of which s are males and s are females. One family will then make s matings and produce s families in the next generation. Each mating is, by hypothesis, of a brother with his sister. Starting as before with a pair from a population in which all individuals are of constitution Aa there will be in the next generation one family of the AA + Aa+aA- aa type. In all succeeding generations there will be six types of families, viz.: (1) AA families. (2) AA + Aa families. (3) Aa families. (4) Aa + 2Aa-+ aa families. (5) Aa\+ aa families (6) aa families. 1 Papers from the ee Laboratory of the Maine Agricultural Ex- periment Station, No. It seems dieiis ny pe as a general series of ‘í Studies on In- ts o hi sis of the Problem of Inbreeding,’’ AMER. Nat., Vol. XLVII, pp. 577- 615, 1913. II. ‘‘Tables for Calculating Coefficients of» Inbreeding,’’ Ann. Rept. Me. Agr. Expt. Sta. for 1913, pp. 191-202. III. ‘‘On the Results of Inbreeding a Mendelian Population: A Correction and oa of Previous Conclusions, ”? AMER. Nart., Vol. XLVIII, pp. 57-62, 2 AMER. Nat., Vol. XVLIII, pp. 57-62, 1914. 491 492 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII The proportionate number of each of these types of families | will change in successive generations according to the following system. Let on denote the number of AA families in the n— ith generation, and Pn. denote the number of AA-+ Aa families in the n— 1th generation, and : Qn, denote the number of Aa families in the n— ith generation, and Yn, denote the number of AA and 2Aa and aa families in the n— 1th generation, and Un_, denote the number of Aa- aa families, and Un_, denote the number of aa families. It will be possible to write down u and v in any case without calculation because of the symmetrical relations of a Mendelian population, since always under normal conditions such as are assumed in the general treatment, we have Un. = Pn-1 Un- TED On-1; Un = Pn, Un = On It is necessary, therefore, to consider only the coefficients for the first four types of family. In the nth generation the consti- tution of the population in respect of families (not individuals) will be as follows: Families in nth generation ` =S (0n + 1/4pn +1/16r,_,) AA families + s(1/2pn_,+ 1/4rn) AA + Aa families + 8(1/8rn_,) Aa families a 8(1/2pn_. + Qn-1 + 1/41.) AA + 2Aa-+ aa families + s(un)Aa-+t aa families j+ s(n) aa families. Or, taking coefficients alone we have = Ona +1/4pn_,+1/16rn_,, Pn=1/2pn_, + 1/4tn_1, Gn aoe 1/ Brn, n= 1/2pn- + Ina +1/ ea Un = 1/2tty:, + 1/40n4 = Un = Vna + 1/4tn + Wik. A (i) No.572] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 493 III. Let us see how this formula works out in a concrete case. Assume the same conditions of fertility as in the former paper, that is, put 2s = 32, or s=16. Start with a single AA + 2Aa + aa family. Then Cig, SD, Pn-1 rs Qn-1 = 0, fas Then in the next generation we shall have 16{0 + 1/4(0) + 1/16(1)}=1AA family + 16{1/2(0) +1/4(1)}—4(AA+ Aa) families + 16{1/8(1) } =2Aa families + 16{1/2(0) +0+1/4(1)}= 4(AA +2Aa-+aa) families + 4(Aa-+ aa) families + laa family. This is the fact. In the next generation we shall have 16{1 + 1 +1/16(4)} 364A families + 16{1/2(4) + 1/4(4) }— 48 (AA + Aa) families + 16{1/8 (4) } — 84a families + 16{1/2(4) + 2 + 1/4(4) }=—= 80 (4A + 24a + aa) families + 48(Aa + aa) families - +36 (aa) families. — This is the fact. In the next generation we shall have 16{36 + 1/4(48) + 1/16 (80) } —16 X 53 — 8484A families + 16{1/2(48) + 1/4(80) } 16 X 44=704(AA + Aa) familie + 16{1/8(80) } —160Aa families + 16{1/2(48) + 8 + 1/4(80) } 16 X 52=—832(AA + 24a + aa) families + 704(Aa-+ aa) families + 848aa families, Succeeding oe follow the same law and need not be worked out in IV. So far ae dietaniGn has confined itself to families, as this must be the basic unit in the theory of any form of inbreeding. Turning to individuals we have the following simple relations to pass to individuals. In the nth generation the number of 494 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST — [Vou. XLVIII AA (or aa) individuals = 2s (0n) H- s(pn) + 1/28 (ra), Aa (or aA) individuals = 2s (qn) + s(1/2pn) +1/2s(1n). The first of the above expressions multiplied by 2 gives the total homozygotes, and the second multiplied by 2 gives the total heterozygotes. RAYMOND PEARL PARALLEL MUTATIONS IN ŒNOTHERA BIENNIS L. In the summer of 1912 I cultivated pure strains of O. biennis L. and of the O. biennis cruciata de Vr. of our Dutch dunes, as well as of their hybrids, made with the purpose of studying the behavior of the cruciata-character in crosses. In one of these cultures I unexpectedly obtained two mutants, which because of their similarity to corresponding variants derived from O. Lamarckiana have been called O. biennis nanella and O. biennis semi-gigas. The first mutant, O. biennis nanella, occurred in the second generation of the cross O. biennis X O. biennis cruciata and. differed from O. biennis in all those points which separate O. Lamarckiana nanella from O. Lamarckiana. The other vari- ant, O. biennis semi-gigas, appeared in the second generation of the reciprocal cross, O. biennis cruciata X O. biennis, suggesting immediately by its much more vigorous habit and especially by the larger size of its buds and flowers the differences between O. Lamarckiana and O. gigas. A count of its diploid number of chromosomes proved it to deserve the name semi-gigas, 21 chromosomes being shown by nuclear plate-stages in the meris- tematic tissue of young buds. From these facts, showing that O. biennis is in a mutating condition, I drew the conclusion that the phenomenon of mutation in the genus @nothera is older than the species O. Lamarckiana—O. biennis generally being consid- ered to be an older species than O. Lamarckiana—and further, that the mutations in this group can not be the result of hybridi- zation, as was assumed by some authors at that time—nobody doubting of the purity and constancy of O. biennis. As a mat- ter of fact, both of my mutants have been derived from crosses between O. biennis and O. biennis cruciata. But I laid special emphasis on the fact that O. biennis and O. biennis cruciata have exactly the same germinal constitution except for the factors that determine the shape of the petals, O. biennis cruciata being prob- No.572] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 495 ably a mutant from O. biennis itself. Therefore, hybrids between these two forms can be looked upon as pure O. biennis except for floral characters. With this conception Bradley Moore Davis does not agree.’ He thinks that the O. biennis and O. biennis cruciata of our dunes are not so closely related types, that a cross between them can be treated ‘‘as though it were the combination of forms within the same species which have similar germinal constitutions.’’ He says: It should be made clear that the form “ O. biennis cruciata” is recog- nized in the more recent taxonomic treatments as a true species sharply distinguished from types of biennis by its floral characters. ... O cruciata is found wild in certain regions of New ean and New York and is consequently a native American specie . Whatever may have been the origin of O. cruciata or its Haat HisHionehip to O. biennis, a cross between these types must certainly be regarded as a cross between two very distinct evolutionary lines and its product a hybrid in which marked modifications of germinal constitution are to be expected. From Davis’s point of view I ‘‘really made a cross between two rather closely related species’’ and obtained in the second generation ‘‘two marked variants due to some germinal modifica- tions as the result of the cross.’’ In so far as my observations bear upon the problem of mutation Davis’s interpretation is exactly the reverse of mine. To him they further illustrate the same phenomenon which he is obtaining through his ‘‘hybrids of biennis and grandiflora, namely, that behavior by which these hybrids in the F, generation throw off variants that in taxonomic practise would be considered new species readily Gieree piano from the parents of the cross and from the F, hybrid.’’ It will be shown in the following lines that the objections made by Davis are not sufficiently justified. My argument consists of two points, In the first place, Davis is mistaken as to the nature of the O. biennis cruciata de Vr. of our dunes. This strain is in reality quite another type than the different forms of the American 0. cruciata Nutt., called by some authors O. biennis cruciata. With this species it has in common only the character of the narrow 1 Bradley Moore Davis, ‘‘ Mutation in @nothera biennis L.?’? THE AMERI- CAN Naturauist, Vol. XLVII, 1913, pp. 116-121; ‘‘Genetical Studies on Enothera,’? IV, THE AMERICAN Naturauist, Vol. XLVII, 1913, pp. 546-571. 496 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII petals, all other features of the stem, foliage, flowerspikes and fruits being exactly those of the Dutch O. biennis L. It must certainly be looked upon as a mutation from the O. biennis L. of our sand dunes. Until now it has only been found a couple of times in single individuals in the midst of the ordinary O. biennis, the first time in 1900 by Dr. Ernst de Vries in the dunes in the neighborhood of Santpoort, Holland, in one individual—and from this one specimen all the subsequent generations of O. bien- ms cruciata in the cultures grown by de Vries and by myself have been derived. Besides this, our O. biennis and O. biennis cruciata are so similar to one another except for floral structure that plants of both types can not be separated before the flowers open. Therefore we have the right to assume that the crossing of these two forms is concerned alone with the floral characters and that with respect to all other characters parents as well as hybrids are mere biennis. Therefore the two variants which arose in my cul- tures from crosses between O. biennis and O. biennis cruciata obviously prove the faculty of mutation in O. biennis. In the second place I have found now that it is not necessary to cross O. biennis with O. biennis cruciata in order to obtain the above named mutants, as Davis seems to believe. Already in his new book Professor de Vries figures a dwarf derived from O. biennis cruciata grown in pure line. Shortly afterwards I myself obtained six mutants from the O. biennis of our sand dunes grown also in pure line. A few details about these cultures of last year may be given here. In all they counted 920 individuals, 430 of which belonged to the third and 490 to the fourth generation of a pure line, the point of departure for which had been one in- dividual brought into the experimental garden in the rosette stage from the dunes near Wyk aan Zee in the beginning of 1905 and self-fertilized in the same year. The six mutants which ap- peared in these pure cultures of O. biennis were the following. First a dwarf, then a biennis semi-gigas having 21 chromosomes and finally four individuals of the O. biennis sulfurea, a pale- flowered form of O. biennis, which had been found already several times in our dunes in the midst of the ordinary biennis, but was not with certainty known to be a mutant from the latter form until now. The two first named mutants and one sulfurea ap- peared in the third generation of our pure line, the nanella and the semi-gigas coming from the same mother. The three remain- ing sulfwrea-individuals appeared in the fourth generation, all No. 572] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 497 descending from the same motherplant. Of these mutants the nanella and semi-gigas are especially valuable because similar forms have been produced by O. Lamarckiana. It will be seen that the biennis-dwarfs seem to be somewhat rarer than the dwarfs of Lamarckiana. Whilst for the latter the mutation coefficient is about 1 per cent. our O. biennis nanella appeared as the only dwarf among 920 individuals. The above cited O. biennis cruciata nanella was the only dwarfed individual in a culture of 500. And the dwarf which I got in 1911 was the only one among about 600 plants. In this connection I wish to recall the conclusion reached by de Vries that in O. Lamarckiana the pangen for tall stature must be assumed to be present in the labile condition on both sides, in O. biennis, however, only in the male sexual type, whilst in the female sexual type active alta-pangens have to be supposed. The way from biennis to biennis nanella might there- fore possibly be somewhat longer than the one from Lamarckiana to Lamarckiana nanella. The biennis semi-gigas which appeared in the last summer corresponded in all points exactly with the mutant of 1911. Moreover a count of the chromosomes, as shown ‘by nuclear plate-stages in the meristematic tissue of young buds, determined them to be 21 in number. Even as the specimen of this type, that appeared in 1911, and as the semi-gigas mutants produced by O. Lamarckiana, the plant of last year proved to be almost absolutely sterile. In his second above-mentioned paper Davis says about i 0. biennis of our dunes: ‘‘No species of @nothera is perhaps so free from suspicion as to its gametic purity. If Stomps can obtain mutations from tested material of the Dutch biennis grown in pure lines he will have the basis of a strong argument, Fortunately the experiment asked for by Davis, has been had in the same year as his criticism. The Dutch biennis L., culti- vated in pure line, has produced a dwarf, a semi-gigas acd some sulfurea-individuals, proving its mutability beyond all doubt. I therefore trust that the conclusions arrived at in my first paper, concerning this mutability and its consequences, may now be ac- cepted as thoroughly valid. THEO. J. Stomps AMSTERDAM, HOLLAND 498 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. XLVIII IN a recent review’ of Stomps’s studies on @nothera biennis L.? from the sand dunes of Holland I protested against his desig- nating as mutants a nanella type and a semi-gigas type which were obtained in the second generation of crosses between Eno- thera biennis Linneus and its variety O. biennis cruciata de Vries. The criticism was presented on the general ground that however close the possible relationships between the two parent forms, they nevertheless constituted lines so far apart as to render unsafe a conclusion that marked variants obtained from their crossing are mutants in the sense of de Vries and Stomps. Such variants, it seemed to me, might have been the result of _ hybridism between two lines sufficiently divergent to upset the similarity of germinal constitution shown in their vegetative morphology, for the species biennis and its variety cruciata are said to differ only in their flower structure. In that review I incorrectly associated O. biennis cruciata de Vries with O. cruciata Nutt., an American species entirely dis- tinct from the variety cruciata of de Vries, which has been found only once (in the year 1900) on the sand dunes of Holland among plants of O. biennis. I greatly regret my confusion of these two types, since I was led in my criticism to regard Stomps’s crosses between biennis and biennis cruciata as though they were crosses between two distinct although possibly closely related species. In this I was clearly mistaken, since all of the evidence short of experimental proof, which Stomps may yet obtain, indi- cates that biennis cruciata de Vries is a variety of biennis L. and arose as a mutation on the sand dunes of Holland. The crosses of Stomps are, therefore, to be regarded as between a species and its mutant variety. I trust that the mutationists will accept this acknowledgment of an error. There is, I believe, a body of naturalists for whom the value of evidence for mutation rests fundamentally upon the unques- tioned purity of the parent stock, and to them any cross, no matter how close, is open to criticism. Stomps has justified his first con- clusions by obtaining in later studies the same mutants biennis nanella and biennis semi-gigas from lines of the pure species O. biennis Linneus. Had he waited for these later results before 1 Davis, B. M., ‘‘Mutations in @nothera biennis L.?’’ AMERICAN NAT- URALIST, Vol. XLVII, p. 116, 1913. 2 Stomps, T. J., ‘‘ Mutation bei @nothera biennis L.,’’ Biol. Centralb., Vol. XXXII, p. 521, 1912. No. 572] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 499 publishing on the first there could have been no objections to his main contention that O. biennis from the sand dunes of Holland is capable of giving rise to true mutants. Stomps is continuing his studies on this same Dutch biennis with the view of determining its possible powers of mutation, and it is a pleasure to review his second paper? which presents some extremely interesting data, a paper in which no important criti- cism can be based on the source and character of the material em- ployed. No wild species of evening primrose has been so long under experimental and field observation or is better known to the workers with cnotheras than this plant. The species has proved uniform in culture to a remarkable degree and it would be difficult to find a type of @nothera so free from suspicion of gametic purity. The species appears to have been in Holland since pre-Linnwan days and is therefore very old. As material for experimental studies on mutation the Dutch biennis seems to the writer the best of all the œnotheras so far brought into the experimental garden. The starting point of Stomps’s cultures of @nothera biennis was a plant transplanted from the sand dunes in 1905. From seed of this plant, self-pollinated, a second generation was grown in 1910, three selfed plants of which gave the seed for a third generation of 430 individuals, and a fourth generation of 490 plants was grown from two selfed plants of the third generation. Thus in all 930 individuals were observed in the third and fourth generations from the plant that gave rise to these pure lines. It is true that these lines have not been under selection for many generations, but, considering the stability of the species and its habit of close pollination, it is very improbable that the source of the cultures should have been a plant not representative of the type. Furthermore, Stomps presumably will continue indefi- nitely the lines now established and thus determine through later generations whether their mutating habits remain constant. Among the 430 plants of the third generation there appeared 1 biennis nanella, 1 biennis semi-gigas and 1 individual of biennis sulfurea; the first two came from the same mother plant. Among the 490 plants of the fourth generation appeared 3 individuals of biennis sulfurea, all from the same selfed mother. The variety sulfurea differs from the species biennis in having flowers of a 3 Stomps, T. J., ‘‘Parallele Mutationen bei Gnothera biennis L.,’’ Ber. deut. bot. Gesell., Vol. XXXII, p. 179, 1914. 500 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII lighter yellow, and is reported by de Vries to be not uncommon in the wild state mixed with the species proper. Sulfurea has been held systematically to be a variety of biennis but this is the first time that it has appeared in the experimental garden as a derivative of that species. Thus out of a total of 920 plants there were 4 individuals of the color variety sulfwrea, 1 nanella and 1 semi-gigas, in all 6 mutants, a showing that may well gratify Stomps. The mutant biennis nanella differed from typical biennis in much the same way that Lamarckiana nanella differs from Lamarckiana and like the latter dwarf showed evidence of a bacterial infection. Certain selfed flowers set no seed because of diseased stigmas. Other flowers pollinated from pure biennis set good fruit. The ratio of the appearance of biennis nanella is much lower than the mutation coefficient of one per cent. which de Vries has reported for Lamarckiana nanella. It should also be remembered that de Vries* obtained a cruciata nanella in a culture of 500 plants from O. biennis cruciata. “The mutant biennis semi-gigas in comparison with typical biennis showed a stronger habit, broader leaves, thicker buds, larger flowers, supernumerary stigma lobes, and the presence of 4-cornered pollen grains. Counts of the chromosomes in meristematic tissue determined the number to be 21. Therefore in this plant, as in the biennis semi-gigas obtained by Stomps from the cross cruciata X biennis, there is clear cytological evi- dence that one of the gametes which formed the zygote contained 14 chromosomes, i. e., double the number characteristic of the gametes of Gnothera. This is another case of triploid mutants in @nothera to be added to the list of Stomps and Miss Lutz. The plant was self sterile, but set fruit when pollinated by biennis, although the yield of seed was very poor. Stomps is justified in calling attention to the agreement of his second biennis nanella with the plant derived in 1911 from the cross biennis X cruciata, and of the agreement of his second biennis semt-gigas with the plant from the cross cruciata X biennis. It is to be hoped that he will next obtain the cruciata variety as a direct mutant from the Dutch biennis and thus establish its relationship and origin beyond all possible doubt. _ Stomps has before him the opportunity of making through the study of Gnothera biennis very important contributions to our 4 See ‘‘Gruppenweise Artbildung,’’ p. 299 and Fig. 108. No. 572] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 501 knowledge of the frequency of mutations and their importance in organic evolution. That retrogressive mutations take place is not likely to be seriously doubted by any one who has followed the experimental work of recent years both botanieal and zoological. The loss of characters through germinal modification, even in what seem to be ‘‘pure lines,’’ appears to be not uncommon. Most of all is desired information on the possibilities, fre- queney and character of progressive mutations. Can the muta- tion theory satisfactorily explain progressive advances in organic evolution or must amphimixis chiefly carry that responsibility ? -~ Mutants of the tetraploid gigas-like type would appear to be pro- gressive, and we can see the reason in their doubled chromosome count which gives larger nuclei, larger cells and modified tissues. Gigas-like forms are however very rare and in O. Lamarckiana gigas the fertility is relatively low. More common are the trip- loid semi-gigas forms, but these seem to be sterile or almost sterile when selfed, and the work of Geerts indicates that the triploid number in @nothera returns to the normal through the elimination of supernumerary chromosomes. Very inter- esting is the recent paper of Gates and Thomas*® which offers evidence that lata-like characters are associated with the pres- ence of a single additional chromosome. And what of the series of forms which differ from the @nothera parent types with as yet no evidence of peculiarities in their chromosome count, brevistylis, levifolia, rubrinervis, obo- vata, scintillans, ete. Will forms similar to these and perhaps others in addition be represented in a series of derivatives from @nothera biennis? The mutants biennis nanella and biennis sulfurea belong to this group and have already been obtained by Stomps. One may almost envy him his opportunity for an intensive study of this species. BRADLEY Moore Davis UNIVERSITY OF PENNSYLVANIA, June, 1914 5 Gates, R. R., and Thomas, N., ‘‘A Cytological Study of Gnothera mut. lata and @. sinks semilata in Relation to Mutation,’’ Quart. Jour. Mic. Soi., Vol. LIX, p.°523, 1914. 502 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vor. XLVII THE THEORETICAL DISTINCTION BETWEEN MULTIPLE ALLELOMORPHS AND CLOSE LINKAGE PROFESSOR CASTLE’S difficulty in understanding the distinction made by Mr. Dextert is owing to his unfamiliarity at first hand with the phenomenon of linkage. The distinction between allelo- morphs and close linkage has already been given several times elsewhere and need not be repeated; but if Professor Castle has failed to note it, or to see its significance, it is probable that others may have done the same. I may be pardoned, therefore, for attempting once more to show why, for clear thinking, it is important to keep in mind the difference between allelomorphs and close linkage. Furthermore, since we have here one of the newest developments of Mendelism, it seems to me that it may be worth while not to let Professor Castle’s criticism pass un- challenged. Dexter pointed out that the mode of treatment that Nabours followed in the analysis of his results is the procedure of multiple allelomorphism, although Nabours does not seem entirely con- versant with the fact, but treats the results as though they were regular phenomena. In one case, however, Nabours got an un- conformable individual. Dexter points out that if this case is not due to non-disjunction (a known process that will cover such cases) it shows that here at least the factors involved are not allelomorphs, but must be treated as though closely linked. How could the matter be put more directly? I confess I am at somewhat of a loss to discover why Professor Castle is con- fused. Perhaps it is the subsequent development of Dexter’s explanation that has troubled him. Let us again try to make the distinction clear. ` If the factors B and E are not allelomorphic to each other then each must have another allelomorph. This is nothing but pure Mendelism, which no one will, I suppose, dispute. It is entirely irrelevant whether we use small letters or none at all (as Castle prefers) for the allelomorphs. If they are a part of the Men- delian machinery, who cares very much what we call them? If then we have here two pairs of allelomorphs, crossing over may take place, as it does in other cases where two pairs of linked 1 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST, June, 1914, p. 383. No.572] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 503 genes are involved.? This is all there is to the matter. We need not dwell, therefore, at length on Professor Castle’s statement that here is another case of an erroneous conclusion reached in consequence of using small letters for ‘‘ absent’’ characters, except to remark that Dexter did not use small letters for absent characters, and that the erroneous conclusion has been drawn by Professor Castle himself. T. H. MORGAN COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY PRoFEssorR MorGAN has called my attention to the fact that in criticizing a single point in Mr. Dexter’s review I have given the impression, to some at least, that I regarded Dexter’s views as erroneous. Such was not my intention, and I wish to correct the impression, if I may. I do not for a moment question the reality of ‘‘unit-character’’ inheritance’ or indorse the idea of ‘‘the or- ganism as a whole’’ as the only inheritance unit. I agree here en- tirely with the view which I understand Dexter to hold. If Na- bours has encountered nothing but simple allelomorphs among his grasshoppers (which I neither assert nor deny), this by no means proves that only simple allelomorphs exist even among said grass- hoppers. An organism which seems to have only one variable ““gene’’? may nevertheless possess any number of other genes which are not varying so far as we can discover, and in which con- sequently all zygotes are homozygous and all gametes similar to each other. It is only in Dexter’s discussion of the significance of the ex- ceptional ‘‘B E T” individual that I should dissent from any part of his excellent review. Nabours’s explanation of this case, ac- cording to Dexter, is essentially that of ‘‘non-disjunction,’’ in- stead of which Dexter himself offers the explanation of ‘‘link- age,’ and proposes a repetition of the experiment to decide between them. Now I do not question for a moment the genuine- ness of either ‘‘non-disjunction’’ or ‘‘linkage,’’ as they occur for example in Drosophila. Through the kindness of Professor Mor- gan I have been able to demonstrate both these phenomena re- peatedly to classes in geneties in the course of their laboratory work upon Drosophila. The point which I wished to make in com- 2 Crossing over would not take place if the factors in question were allelo- morphic. If the ease is one of non-disjunction the subsequent generation would also give a different kind of result from that of linkage. (See Bridges, Jour. Exp. Zool., 1913.) 504 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII menting on Dexter’s review (and this is the only point in which ` I dissent from his opinions) is that the repetition of the experi- ment, provided it had the outcome suggested by Dexter, would leave us as much in the dark as we were before concerning the correct interpretation of the result. Very likely, however, addi- tional facts might be observed which would give some clue, so that I quite agree with Dexter’s suggestion that the case should receive further study. But I can not see that at present linkage has more in its favor as an interpretation than non-disjunction. The ‘‘demonstration’’ which Mr. Dexter gave of his argument by introducing duplicate ‘‘symbols’’ instead of the single set used by Nabours, seemed to me quite superfluous and possibly to have been a real stumbling block in the logical process. This is why I raised the question as to the significance of the small letters. The terminology is that of the ‘‘presence-absence’’ hypothesis, as commonly understood; but Professor Morgan assures me that such is not the significance which Dexter attaches to the symbols used. It seems to me therefore that the significance attached to the symbols is vital to the argument in the ‘‘demonstration.’ I quite agree with Professor Morgan, however, that symbols are a matter of small consequence. Suppose we omit the ‘‘dem- onstration’’ by means of symbols altogether. Should we then have any reason to favor linkage as an interpretation rather than non-disjunction? I can not see that we should have. It seems to me quite possible that neither explanation will prové adequate. When albino mammals are crossed with colored ones, piebalds sometimes are obtained in later generations. So far as we know, these result neither from ‘‘non-disjunction’’ nor from ‘‘cross- overs.’’ Perhaps the B E I individual also is a ofaa quid. W. E. CASTLE NOTES AND LITERATURE BIOMETRICS An ImporTANT CONTRIBUTION TO STATISTICAL THEORY ONE of Pearson’s most valuable contributions to statistical theory is his test for goodness of fit.t It enables one, with the aid of Elderton’s? tables, easily to determine the probability that a given system of observed frequencies does or does not differ significantly from a series of theoretical frequencies supposed to graduate the observations. The significance of this criterion in Mendelian work has recently been pointed out by Harris.* Hitherto this criterion has found an important limitation in the fact that, as originally developed by Pearson, it was appli- eable only to frequency systems. It could be used to test good- ‘ness of fit only where the observations were counts of the number of times particular classes of events occurred. But, of course, frequency systems comprise only one kind of observational data to which one has occasion to fit curves. Much more often there is need for a criterion of goodness of fit where the observations are of the nature of true ordinates, rather than frequencies. Such cases include all data of the sort where a mean y is deter- -mined for each x, as in a growth curve; or in the regression observed in a correlation table, where for each successive value of one of the variables the mean value of the correlated variable is caleulated. There has been no method of testing the good- ness of fit for such curves. From a visual inspection of the . plotted regression line one has been compelled to form his judg- ment as to whether it was or was not a good fit. Recently a Russian statistician, E. Slutsky,* has extended 1 Pearson, K., ‘‘On the Criterion that a Given System of Deviation from the Probable in the Case of a Correlated System of Variables is Such that it Can be Reasonably Supposed to Have Arisen from Random Sampling,’’ Phil. Mag., 5th Series, Vol. L, pp. 157-175, 1900. : 2 Biometrika, Vol. I, pp. 155-163. 3 Harris, J. A., ‘‘A Simple Test of the Goodness of Fit of Mendelian Ratios,’? AMER. Nat., Vol. 46, 1912, pp. 741-745, 1912. t Slutsky, E., ‘‘On the Criterion of Goodness of Fit of the Regression ion and on the Best Method of Fitting Them to the Data,’’ Jour. Roy. Stat. e Vol. LXXVII, Part I (December, 1913), issued 1914, pp. 78-84. 505 506 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vor. XLVIII Pearson’s theory to cover the class of curves, formerly not amenable to such test. The result forms an extremely valuable extension of biometric theory. Briefly Slutsky’s essential result may be put as follows. He finds (the complete proof is not given in this paper) that 2 nN, € aeae) N 2 ’ Tnxpy where x” is the quantity denoted by the same letter in Pearson’s original work, and is the argument in Elderton’s table; Nen is the frequency in the zp array, i. e., the number of observations on which each observed ordinate is based; ep is the difference between the observed and the calculated mean y for each £p array; ando,, is the standard deviation of each x» array; îi. e., the standard deviation of the group of observations from which each particular y was calculated. S, as usual, denotes summa- tion. Knowing x’, P is read directly from Elderton’s tables. Slutsky gives a couple of examples of the application of the method in his paper. For illustration here I have preferred to take an example from my own unpublished data. The observa- tions (y,,,) in this case are the mean butter productions of American Jersey cattle, based on seven-day tests The theoretical points Y,,, are calculated from the equation, y = 14.21098 + 02500 — 003822 + 3.0104 log a, the constants of which were determined from the observations by the method of least squares. The test for goodness of fit is carried out in Table I. It should be said that, following the suggestion given by Slutsky in his paper, I have used in the o,,, column the graduated rather than the observed values. In the present case the scedastie curve is hopelessly far from a straight line. It is, in point of fact, logarithmic. From this table we have x? = 32.115. This is beyond the range of Elderton’s table. By a rough, but sufficiently noona, graph- ical TOEA, I find for present values of n’ and x’, P= .417 about. In other words, if the butter production of Jersey cows changes with age according to the curve given, we should expect to 5 For data see ‘‘ Jersey Sires and Their a Daughters,’’ published by American Jersey Cattle Club, New York, 1 No. 572] > NOTES AND LITERATURE 507 get a worse agreement between observation and theory in 42 out of every 100 random samples on which the point was tested. In other words, the fit may be considered sufficiently good. As a matter of fact, the fit is extraordinarily close over most of the curve. Four (only) out of the 32 ordinates contribute more than 50 per cent. of the value of x°. TABLE I Age in Observed Cale, Butter Standard Years |Butter Produc-| Production Errors | Frequency! Dev. of tion in Lbs. in Lbs. Arrays 7 C= yyy Tk Yy 5 “Pp (Yey z p ) ea Inep g "nrp 1.25 14.25 14.23 .02 2 500 1.75 15.15 15.15 00 2.25 15.57 15.69 AZ 273 1.49 1.771 2.75 15.96 16.06 10 312 on | 932 3.25 16.38 16.35 .03 545 2.07 | 114 3.75 16.72 16.57 -15 511 2.40 | R21 4.25 16.92 16.74 18 04 2.38 4.027 4.75 17.09 16.89 20 532 2.49 3.432 5.25 17.01 17.00 .01 556 2.56 5.75 17.07 17.09 .02 382 2.62 022 6.25 16.98 17.16 18 419 65 1.933 6.75 17.04 17.21 “ae 277 2.68 1.114 7.25 17.09 17.25 16 85 2.68 1.016 7.75 17.48 17.27 21 190 2.68 1.167 5 17.30 17.28 -02 66 2.67 8.75 Was ve 17.27 -10 121 174 9.25 17.56 17.25 31 109 2.61 1.515 16.67 17.21 54 95 2.57 10.25 17.05 17.17 12 2.52 143 10.75 17.42 17.11 39 619 11.25 16.95 17.05 10 2.40 11.75 .00 16.97 28 33 005 12.25 17.05 6.88 17 20 2.26 113 12.75 16.54 16.79 25 7 2.18 09: 13.25 16.34 6.68 34 11 2.09 .291 13.75 18.14 16.56 1.58 9 1.99 5.673 14.25 15.89 44 7 1.88 .599 14.75 16.15 16.30 15 5 1.77 036 15.25 16.37 16.16 21 4 1.65 065 15.75 15.75 00 2 1.53 053. 16.25 15.42 15.84 42 3 1.40 Boys 16.75 15.75 15.67 08 4 1.27 016 Totals... 5,781 32.115 It may be said, in conclusion, that Slutsky’s contribution is one which will be highly valued by all investigators who have a critical interest in the graduation of observational data, whatever the field in which they may be working. RAYMOND PEARL 08 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST . [Vou. XLVIII A NEW MODE OF SEGREGATION IN GREGORY’S TETRAPLOID PRIMULAS IN a recent paper! Gregory reports a very interesting case in which two different races of Primulas suddenly gave rise to giant tetraploid forms, having double the usual number of chromo- somes, and apparently having the factors doubled also (individu- ally), for this was true of all the factors which could be followed in his hybridization experiments. It is important to know how segregation will take place in such individuals, as there are four -allelomorphs of each gene present. Let us suppose that a tetraploid form pure for the dominant AA pure recessive giant (22). Gametes AA and aa will meet in factor A (ana therefore of composition — =) is crossed with a fertilization, forming the hybrid à = (the maternally derived genes are represented on one line, say the upper, the paternally derived genes on the other line). Now, if this were an ordinary case of ‘‘multiple factors’’? in a diploid organism, although the two dominant factors, which we may again call A’s, may produce the same effect upon the organ- ism, yet they are not interchangeable, and the same is true of the recessive factors. That is, if we call both dominants A, we must designate one of them as At, and the corresponding recessives must also be designated as a and a’, for A will always segregate into a different gamete from a, and At from a’, there being two distinct allelomorphic pairs. On the chromosome view of he- redity, we would say that A and a always lay opposed to each other, in homologous chromosomes, on the spindle of the reduction division, as did also A‘ and a‘, but neither A nor a lay in chromosomes homologous to those of either A’ or a’, and assorted independently of them. The line-up of factors on the spindle in the reduction division in this case would be i equally likely to bes = or ai depending merely upon which 1R. P. Gregory, ‘‘On the Genetics of Tetraploid Plants in Primula sinensis,’’ Proceedings of the Royal Society, 1914. 2%, e, a case where two (or more) independent pairs of factors produce similar effects, upon the same character. Many examples of this are known, e. g., the inheritance of red flower in flax. No. 572] NOTES AND LITERATURE 509 way the pairs are turned with reference to each other. The first alignment gives gametes AA! and aa’, the second gives Aat and aA*. Thus three gametes with a dominant factor to one pure recessive would on the average be produced, the ratio being 1AA:2Aa:1laa, omitting primes. In a tetraploid form, however, A and A? are alike and inter- 1 changeable, as also are a and a‘. In the hybrid fara’ therefore, there would be at least one other mode of pairing of allelomorphs possible, giving two new modes of line-up on the reduction spindle, and they would occur just as frequently as the two previous kinds. The two new arrangements would be z3 x giving gametes AA! = ata, like those in the first of the two previous cases, and ai Do giving gametes Aa and atA1.° These latter gametes would be indistinguishable from the Aat and aA? gametes given by the second of the two usual arrangements un- less A could be distinguished from A‘ and a from at. This could happen only if the allelomorphs were of four different kinds or if there were linkage of these genes with other genes for which the plant was heterozygous. Unless, therefore, linkage or mul- tiple allelomorphism were involved, we could not distinguish between this mode of pairing of allelomorphs and the usual kind; both would give three gametes containing at least one dominant, to one pure recessive (i. e., LAA: 2Aa:laa, omitting primes). Still a third type of pairing of allelomorphs is possible ina tetraploid plant, however. There seems no a priori reason, on the chromosome view, why, in a tetraploid plant, a gene should have to segregate from one of the allelomorphs derived from the opposite parent. That is, in a plant of composition — AE paternally derived genes being indicated on, say, the upper hae maternally derived ones on the lower, there is no apparent yy why the line-up of chromosomes at reduction should not be -y At 5 a or as often as it is one of the other types, since all four chromosomes are homologous. Thus we should get gametes Aa, Atat, Aa! and Ata 3 If linkage with other genes could be followed, we should with this mode of pairing obtain crossing over between the chromosomes containing A and a‘, respectively, and between those containing At and a, respectively; this would not occur on any other mode of pairing. 510 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII We could distinguish such gametes individually from those obtained by the ordinary arrangements only if linkage were in- volved, for then we should sometimes obtain results indicating that the chromosomes containing A and A? had crossed over with one another, and so had probably behaved as homologous chromosomes at the reduction division. However, we could also determine whether this mode of pairing occurred or not merely by determining the relative numbers of the different kinds of gametes formed. For, if the third type of pairing occurred, we should obtain 4Aa gametes in addition to the 2AA, 4Aa and 2aa derived from the other two types of pairing. The ratio of gametes would then be five containing a dominant to one reces- sive, there being 1AA:4Aa:laa, as opposed to the ratio 1AA:2Aa: laa obtainable on either of the other modes of segre- gation.* The latter or more usual ratio is the only one considered by Gregory, who apparently takes it for granted that in so far segregation must be of the same sort as in diploid forms. Let us see which ratio is more in accord with his experimental data. As the ratio of offspring in a back-cross is the same as the gametic ratio, it will be seen that a back-cross of S by a reces- sive should give 3A : 1a plant on Gregory’s view, the 3A’s consist- ing of 1 = 25 ee On the other view, a back-cross should result in 5A: 1a, the 5A’s consisting of 1 = = “ae —, On inbreeding an aa 4 A plant, however, owing to the random fertilization of gametes, Gregory’s 3:1 gametie ratio would result in a 15:1 ratio among the offspring (which correspond to F,) and our own 5:1 gametic ratio would give a 35:1 ratio of A toa among the offspring. A summary of his back-crosses of P, heterozygous thrum-eyed plants of the type , <5 to recessive pin-eyed plants F gives the result 61 thrum:6 pin (10:1, as compared to the two ex- pectations 3:1 and 5:1). Among the F, thrums there should 4 Counts of chromosomes in the maturation divisions of the tetraploid plants show that the chromosomes synapsed in pairs, not in groups of four. Synapsis in fours would be, in effect, the same as pairing of the random sort suggested in this paper, so far as any one set of allelomorphs are concerned, but it might give different linkage results. No. 572] NOTES AND LITERATURE 511 i A 7 A ,Aa on Gregory ’s view be aa, a >: on the other view ‘= — a, ie 85 ags 88 Tests of twenty-one F, thrums, by mating them to themselves and also to recessives, showed that there was only one which was : AA ¢ certainly eins and 15 which must have been â 5; (A few gave numbers too small to be significant, and one or two were of doubt- ful composition.) This result is within the limits of probable error on the 4:1, but hardly on the 2:1 expectation. The one F, thrum plant which was of composition AA gave, on back- crossing, 67 thrums: 18 pins, a ratio of 3.7:1, to correspond with Gregory’s 3:1 or my 5:1 expectation. On inbreeding it gave 44 thrums: 2 pins, a ratio of 22:1, to correspond with Gregory’s 15:1 or my 35:1 expectation. The other F, thrums, being of composition 2 Z (aside from the few doubtful ones), gave, on the average, 1 thrum: 1 pin on back-crossing, and 3 thrums: 1 pin on inbreeding; these results would be expected on either view. rosses were also made involving the character green versus red stigma (green being dominant). Here the 2s forms, on back-crossing, gave a total of 114 green: 30 red (3.8:1 instead of 3:1 or 5:1), and on inbreeding they gave 75 green:2 red (37.5: 1 instead of 15:1, as on Gregory’s expectation, or 35:1, on my own). It will be seen that the numbers in the above crosses are too small to be very significant, individually, for a settlement of the question at issue, but if summed up they become more decisive. Thus, a sunimary of the offspring of all back-crosses of the AA ; : f =r form to the recessive gives 242 dominants (A) :54 reces- sives (a), or 4.5:1, as compared with the 3:1 expectation of Gregory and the 5:1 of the view advocated in this paper. Where the dominants among these offspring were tested they were found to consist of 12 = and 15— a 3s compared with the 1:2 expectation of Gregory, and ours of 1:4. Finally, a sum- mary of the cases where AA forms were inbred shows that 119 - dominants:4 recessives resulted, a ratio of 30:1 where Greg- 512 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII ory’s expectation would be 15:1 and our own 35:1. Moreover, the individual records fluctuate in both directions about the ratios to be expected upon our point of view, but practically all vary in the same direction from the expectation of Gregory, namely, in the direction of the other expectation. here is reason, then, to believe that in these Primulas the factors derived from the same parents may segregate from each other as allelomorphs, while allelomorphs derived from opposite parents meanwhile assort at random. For although the allelo- morphs exist in sets of four they must pair’ two by two for segre- gation, as do the chromosomes, and two derived from the same parent may happen to pair with one another. The chance that this should oceur is one third, since there are three possible modes of pairing. Such a result is difficult to explain except on the chromosome view of heredity. It would give ratios different from those theoretically expected by Gregory, but more in accord with his experimental data. The principle upon which our own expectation is founded may be briefly summed up by saying that where more than two factors which are normally allelomorphie to each other are present, the pairing of these allelomorphs with each other preparatory to segregation usually® takes place at random. HERMANN J. MULLER 5 That this is not always true is shown by Bridges’ case of ‘‘non-dis- junctional’’ females of Drosophila, which contain one Y and two X chromo- somes. Any two of these chromosomes normally act as homologues to each other in the reduction division of the normal fly, which contains only two ` of them. But where all three are present together they do not pair at ran- dom, for they oftener undergo the segregation X-XY than XX-~Y, pre- sumably because the two X’s are much more like each other than like the Y, and so more apt to act as homologues. VOL. XLVIII, NO. 573 “ SEPTEMBER, 1914 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST A MONTHLY JOURNAL Devoted to the Advancement of the Biological Sciences with Special Reference to the Factors of Evolution CONTENTS a I. Studies on Inbreeding. Dr. RAYMOND PEAR - z 513 Il. The Chromosome Hypothesis of — sopied to Cases in Sweet Peas and Primul ALVIN B. BRIDG 524 HI. The SER EEEE as E to P = A. H. STURTEVANT - - -535 IV. Pattern dite in Mammals and Birds, Dr, GLOVER M. ALLEN - 550 V. Shorter Articles and Correspondence: The Bearing of the Selection Experi. ments of Castle and = on the TANN of EEREN HERMANN MULLER - = - 567 THE SCIENCE PRESS LANCASTER, PA. GARRISON, N. T. NEW YORK: SUB-STATION 84 The American Naturalist intended for ane soe and books, ete., intended for a should be MSS sent ago Editor of THE AMER articles containing summaries of resea CAN NATURALIST, —— -on-Huds n oe York. wor ing on the problems of Sod cg evolution are especially welcome, ana will be gees sretervies in pen ea reprints of ne per foro are supplied to authors free of charge. Further ‘eprint will be supplied a Subscriptions and adv subbertpiton oes Hed four Canadian posta ertisoments should be sent to the publishers. ollars a year. Foreign postage is fifty cen wenty- -five ‘eis additional. The nts and The arie for single copies is forty cents. Tho: i marefa rates are Four Dollars for a THE SCIENCE PRESS NEW YORK: Sub-Station 84 Lancaster, Pa. Entered as second-class matter, age 2, 1908, at th Garrison, N. Y. Post Office at Lancaster, Pa., under the Act ot Congress of aih 3, 1879. FOR SALE ARCTIC, ICELAND and GREENLAND BIRDS’ SKINS, Well Prepared Low Prices Particulars of G. DINESEN, Bird Collector Husavik, North Iceland, Via Leidle, England WANTED TO PURCHASE a set of BIRDS OF AMERICA by J. J. Audubon, 7 or 8 volumes, please report, stating cash price, stat- ing condition, binding and dates of volumes. F. C. HARRIS, Box 2244 Boston, Massachusetts For Sale Entire An important collection of Indian Birds’ Eggs, containing approximately 3,300 specimens of about 620 species, and in- cluding among other varieties the Green- ish Willow Warbler (Acanthopneuste viridanus), Red-browed Finch (Calla- canthis burtoni), Tibetan Sand-Grouse (Syrrhaptes tibetanus), Spotted Sand- Grouse (Pteroclurus senegallus), Ibis- bill (Ibidorhynchus struthersi), White- bellied Minivet (Pericrocotus erythro- pygius), Bearded Vulture (Gypaëtus barbatus), numerous Cuckoos with their hosts, ete., ete. Particulars and full list may be had from W. F. H. ROSENBERG 57 Haverstock Hill London, N. W. Marine Biological Laboratory Woods Hole, Mass. INVESTIGATION Entire Year Facilities for research in Zoology, nae ag an ee for such a table Courses labora’ instruota of INSTRUCTION Sack N RA om July b E 1 to August 11, 1914 $50.0 Philosophical Aspects of a? aaee Allied Sciences re -e so tn gator = oP Open e ear logi Botanical ace epee ft at information regarding material, address GEO. M. GRAY, Curator, Woods Hole, Maas The annual announcement will be sent on ai = The Director, Marine Biological Laboratory, Woods Hole Mass- THE AMERICAN NATURALIST VoL. XLVIII September, 1914 No. 573 STUDIES ON INBREEDING. V INBREEDING AND RELATIONSHIP COEFFICIENTS ! Dr. RAYMOND PEARL UNIVERSITY OF MAINE In the discussion of inbreeding coefficients contained in a series of recent papers from this laboratory” no mention has been made of an important consideration which arises in connection with such coefficients. The further problem, to which we may now turn, may be stated in the follow- ing way. The pedigree of an individual consists of two halves. One of these halves is made up of the sire and his an- cestors; the other of the dam and her ancestors. Follow- ing the conception of inbreeding set forth in detail in the earlier papers of this series it is plain that the values of the coefficients of inbreeding for a particular pedigree are composed of the following elements. 1. The occurrence of the same individual animals more than once on the sire’s side of the pedigree only. 2. The occurrence of the same individual animals more than once on the dam’s side of the pedigree only. 1 Papers from the oo gaa Laboratory of the Maine Agricultural Ex- periment Station, No. 6 2 Pearl, R., Paa on Inbreeding. I. A Contribution Towards an Analysis of the Problem of Inbreeding,’? AMER. Nat., Vol. XLVII, pp. 577-614, 1913; ‘‘The Measurement of the Intensity of Inbreeding,’’ Me. Agr. Expt. Sta. Bul., 215, pp. 123-138, 1913. Pearl, R., and Miner, J. R., ‘‘ Studies on Inbreeding. III. Tables for Calculating Coefficients of In- breeding,’’ Me. Agr. Expt. Sta. Ann. Rept. for 1913, pp. 191-202, 1913. 513 514 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII 3. The reappearance of animals which appear first on one side of the pedigree (either the sire’s or the dam’s) on the other side. If only 1 and 2 are to be found in the pedigree it means that the sire and the dam are totally unrelated (within the limits covered by the pedigree in the particular case). the other hand, the occurrence of 3 means that sire and dam are in some degree related, and that a portion of the observed inbreeding arises because of that fact. Now the coefficients of inbreeding, in and of themselves, tell nothing about what proportionate part has been played by these three elements in reaching the final result. It is a matter of great importance to have information on this point, because of its genetic significance. It is the pur- pose of this paper to describe a general method for ob- taining this desired information. The first step in the method, stated briefly, is to break up the pedigree elimination table formed to get the suc- cessive values Of pn; — Gnu, in our former notation, into four different parts. One of these parts will include the primary reappearance on the sire’s side of the pedigree of such animals as appear first on the same side. This may be called the ‘‘male only” table. The second part will include the primary reappearance on the dam’s side of such animals as first appear on the same side. This is the ‘‘female only’’ table. The third part will include the primary reappearance on the dam’s side of such animals as first appear on the sire’s side. The fourth part is the reverse of the third. These last two may be called the ‘‘eross tables.’’? The sums of the totals of these partial tables will give the total pn. — qn. Values for the succes- sive generations. The formation of the tables on this plan may be illus- trated with some examples. These examples will also show the skeleton method of writing pedigree elimination tables, which saves much labor. This was referred to, but not significantly illustrated, in the earlier papers. It consists simply in doubling the total of the column for each generation rather than the separate items. No. 573] STUDIES ON INBREEDING TABLE I 515 PARTIAL PEDIGREE La on eh TABLE FOR Kine MELIA Rioter 14TH SHOW- IN RY REAPPEARANCES ON THE SIRE’S THE Ponia OF as WHICH FIRST APPEAR ON THAT SIDE E PRIM SIDE OF Generation 3 4| 5 Melia pond : Son Melia Ann 8d... c.i Lucy’s aoe. Pogis . Melia Ann St. oe tet Letty Rio Allie of es Ginter’: mele Cine 6 68 6 6 eee Pe wpe ete Oe ee ee 8S Ww OSs Pee tow es ve ate Huo’ s iran Vi ae wie be Lord Lisg Lucy of St. Tak Diana of St. Lamberts: Olof. of St. Lambert . Oe eb ele ee kegs 6 ate WLS e E a eee ee ee wey ee E ae eee eee we ete Oa sere eee se sate a ee E Sa la D ee A E, tht we ae se Gee ee we es Cee ee ee ee ere ee Ged eee ee rere tv ele Ce eee ee ee er er) ve we ele stare Pee eee eee et ee we le epee fe wpe te fe see ` d Ce wee ee a A a h E r eo ie, ee a ee E E a E E ae a E Oe ey eee E Rm LA Ga ee . EIE ea Shae gone i). cera few eee Ps ee eee fo ae eee ere thee sprees oa Oe Se ee oe a ae Oe ee Sew eine eel ee wee Pe ee ee ee ee a ee epee ee be ee ee ee ee EAS EN eee ee ee ee ee A Hee ee ge tel ae ee eee ee ee eee ee age e eRe molar ye ns Fae th Ph eee tee Pu es i Fo 8 6 ee ee fae ee ae pescao rw tas Peer ee ee bee ee se ee ede oe oe ee en ae ie, Se ee 6 ee eee ey fee ee ete Ss ee ete see eee P ee E E TAE EAE S ae see eee s.s.s +62 ee S ee eee s.es... se ee ele ‘Te hee ee ieee ss es ee a ed oe We eae | 16 447 | 898 2an S this and au fale Ltd ng table the numb pds the sum of the numbers in the umulated ancestral oe up to the So in question. ers in brackets ar e in each case g column. They represent, the 516 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII # The pedigree for 12 ancestral generations of the Jersey bull King Melia Rioter 14th (103901) may be taken as the first illustration. TABLE II PARTIAL PEDIGREE ELIMINATION TABLE FoR KING MELIA RIOTER 14TH SHOW- GONO ecrini 2 3 4 goteo DA King’s Bioter Lad sonh — — — 1 2 4 8 16 32 64 128 Table III is clearly the one which demands special attention. As will shortly appear, it is the most important for the theory of inbreeding. Let us attempt its analy- sis. Just what does the first entry mean genetically ? Tt states that King Melia Rioter, an animal which first ap- peared on the sire’s side of the pedigree, reappeared in the second ancestral generation on the dam’s side. What this clearly means is that at least one half of all the dam’s ancestors, in the third and higher ancestral generations, are identically the same animals as are ancestors of the T — cno P ih eh ER: $ è “<7 bro & COEFFICIENTS á 4 e 8 s0 a sa GENERATIO Fic. 1. Diagram a (a) Sees ge 1 inbreeing (heavy solid line) and (b) the ehren (heavy broken line) for the Jersey bull, King Melia Rioter To The KGN i order of oa taik and relationship between the sire this case is evident by comparison with the lighter lines, which give the rer it values for continued brother x sister, parent x offspring and cousin reeding. No. 573] STUDIES ON INBREEDING 517 sire. The next entry in Table III indicates that in the fourth and higher ancestral generations at least 5/8 of all the dam’s ancestors were the same individual animals as were also ancestors of the sire. One half of them were the same before the reappearance of St. Lambert’s Rioter King. He makes up the additional 1/8 of the dam’s ancestry. TABLE III PARTIAL PEDIGREE ELIMINATION TABLE FOR KiNG MELIA RIOTER 14TH SHOW- ING THE PRIMARY REAPPEARANCES ON THE DAM’sS SIDE OF THE PEDIGREE OF ANIMALS WHICH FIRST APPEAR ON THE SIRE’S SIDE E a 6/0 sc1s es slala P@ 171 s | 9 (20114) 18 King Melia Rioter......... i1] B Dle E E epe St. Teahibact’s Ristet King: Rs E a bwai E Ra S N. D King of St. Lambert........ te see Oe ep Be oe Aa) oie Renee e o; | CORNO S SO o oo de EE S D N E E are ee St. La: mbert’s Rioter King | oped egies ent GOw a ies Pica cule <> NEE Sota St. Lambart: Boyce... AA E E Sern E Fae sechas Tol eaa aea |1| 2 | 5 | 12 | 28 | 59 | 119 | 240 480/960 1,920 From these tables it is obvious that a very considerable portion of the inbreeding shown in the pedigree of King Melia Rioter 14th arises from the fact that his sire and dam were closely related. Furthermore, both sire and dam are closely inbred in their own lines. The curve of . total inbreeding in this case is shown in Fig. 1, along with the curves for continued brother X sister, parent by off- spring, and cousin X cousin mating. TABLE IV SUMMARIZED PEDIGREE ELIMINATION TABLE FoR Kine MELIA RIOTER 14TH Géneration 2. . 2.5 ii: i846 16) 7 & es Hi a BF oniy. oai. urea 1| 3/16 41|105|219|447| 898| 1,796 S O aa a 3 2; ál Bi 16) S| i 128 Oross-over.. aasa 1/2|/5|12/28| 59|119 |240| 480; 960 1,920 Teste... 4, 2| 6 16 46 104 |232 |475| 959 1,922 | 3,844 From this we have, for = inbreeding coefficients, 518 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII Ze ed Z: == 25,00 Z, == 25.00 P So 2, >=> 50.00 T a OR Ee Ze == S120 Z; = 90.63 D aP Z; == 93.05 Ze == 95.80 Za == 93.85 These facts will possibly be made clearer to those not actually working much with pedigrees by Table V, which gives the first four ancestral generations‘ of the pedigree of King Melia Rioter 14th. Generalizing the above reasoning we get the following result. In A,, and: higher ancestral generations, 2/4 = 50.00 per cent. of the dam’s ancestors are animals which are also ancestors of the sire: In A,, and higher ancestral generations, 5/8 = 62.50 per cent. of the dam’s ancestors are animals which are also ancestors of the sire. In A,, and higher ancestral generations, 12/16 = 75.00 per cent. of the dam’s ancestors are animals which are also ancestors of the sire. In A,, and higher ancestral generations, 28/32 = 87.50 per cent. of the dam’s ancestors are animals which are also ancestors of the sire. In A., and higher ancestral generations, 59/64 = 92.19 per cent. of the dam’s ancestors are animals which are also ancestors of the sire. 4 In the study of pedigrees stress is naturally laid on the ancestral genera- tions, rather than on the filial, as in breeding experiments. It becomes very convenient to have a brief designation for ancestral generations, in the same way that F,, F., ete., are used to denote filial generations. I would suggest the use of the letter A with sub-numbers for this purpose. We then have A, denoting the parental generation, A, the grandparental, A, the great-pa- rental, ete. No. 573] STUDIES ON INBREEDING 519 In Ag, and higher ancestral generations, 119/128 = per cent. of the dam’s ancestors are animals AENG are also ancestors of the sire. In A,, and higher ancestral generations, 240/256 = 93.75 per cent. of the dam’s ancestors are animals which are also ancestors of the sire. In A,,, and higher ancestral generations, 93.75 per cent. of the dam’s ancestors are animals which are also an- cestors of the sire. In A,,, and higher ancestral generations, 93.75 per cent. of the dam’s ancestors afte animals which are also an- cestors of the sire. In A,» and higher ancestral generations, 93.75 per cent. of the dam’s ancestors are animals which are also an- cestors of the sire. TABLE V PEDIGREE FOR Four ANCESTRAL GENERATIONS OF KING MELIA RIOTER 14TH |©. |© |No. 63200 *|No. 56581 No. 2 J | x Melia yoa s Son. b Melia Ann’s King. |No. 100775 9 Marjorie _ Lottie Melia Ann. _ x Melia Ann’s |No. 157263 Q No. 22041 Eg = Son. Le : @ Melia Ann’s Son. = Marjorie Melia faa Ann. No. 9 05883 e Q | S Mary Melia Ann | = No. 181544 9 No. 58169 d'No. 54896 ea | i _ St Lambert’s Rioter King. | A King of All Kings. |No. 114804 Q peoa 5 S Letty St. Lambert’s Letty. $e Silver |No. 148456 Q No. 32559 F Bik Hair. : j _ Exile of St. Anne’s. gn Exile’s Silver olo Hair. No. 60449 2 ie — ae 4th. So |No.73104 "No. 63200 J No. 5658 g ~ ® k y ’s King. = ® Marjorie Melia © Mel elerna oen a Ann’s Son. No. 157263 à y £ @ King Melia - & Marjorie Melia Ann. m Riste. |No. 181544 Q|No. a 2 Q King of All Kings. 3 ® Letty Silver Hair. |No. 1 re) = | & mae s a ive Hair. % No. 219360 2 No: 62098 J |No. 54896 ae a) St. Lambert’s Rioter King. | E King Rioter’s @ St. Lam > | 4 Lad 0. 14 2 mo he ' Dula i King’s Riotress Nora. S iB Riotress No, 218796 Q\No. 57778 m So 9 Maid. St. Lambert's Boy. PE ins: St. Lambert’s No. 174761 9 Zz |e cine ON Rioter Lad’s First Daughter. 520 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII These percentages are quantities of a good deal of interest. They measure the degree in which King Melia Rioter 14th’s sire and dam were related to each other. Community of ancestry is the basis of kinship. Percentages derived in the way shown above, from cross pedigree elimination tables, I propose to call co- efficients of relationship, and to designate by the letter K, with appropriate sub-numbers referring to the genera- tion. These relationship coefficients are, with some limita- tions, independent of the inbreeding coefficients in the values they may take, though the two will usually be cor- related to some degree. It is, however, possible to have a high value of Z with K—v. TABLE VI COMPARING THE MAXIMUM POSSIBLE VALUES OF THE COEFFICIENTS OF IN- BREEDING (Z) WHEN THE COEFFICIENT OF RELATIONSHIP K EQUALs (a) ZERO, AND (b) 100 Generation Maximum Possible Value Maximum Possible Value of Z when K = 0 of Z when K = 100 i 0 0 A, 0 50.00 Ag 50.00 75.00 A, 75.00 87.50 A, 87.50 93.75 As 93.75 96.88 A, 96.88 98.44 As 98.44 99.22 Ay 99.22 99.61 Aw 99.61 99.80 The most important feature of the relationship coeffi- cients is found in their genetic implications. This can be indicated best by an illustration. Let us consider the case of the maximum possible degree of inbreeding with K = 0. This will be found when the sire and the dam are each inbred to the highest possible degree (continued brother X sister mating) but are in no way related to each other. Such a case would be afforded, for example, if a Jersey bull, the product of continued brother X sister mating, was bred to a Holstein cow, which was also the product No. 573] STUDIES ON INBREEDING 521 of a continued brother by sister breeding. Clearly K would be 0, since no animal on one half of the pedigree could even appear on the other. The values of the suc- cessive coefficients of inbreeding (Z’s) in such a case are shown in Table VI, where they are compared with the coefficients of inbreeding in complete continued brother X sister mating, where K = 100.5 From this it appears that an individual may be inbred in 10 generations to within two tenths of one per cent. as intensely, measured by the coefficients of inbreeding, if his sire and dam are in no way related, as he would be if his sire and dam were brother and sister. But clearly the germinal constitution of the individual produced would, except by the most remote chance, be quite different in the two cases. This point is so evident as to need no elab- oration. It has been brought out by East and Hayes.® The values of the K’s for a particular pedigree evi- dently furnish a rough index of the probability that the two germ-plasms which unite to form an individual are alike in their constitution. This will follow because of the fact that the probability of likeness of germinal constitu- tion in two individuals must tend to increase as the num- ber of ancestors common to the two increases. Just what - is the law of this increase in probability is a problem in Mendelian mathematics which has not yet been worked out. The general fact, however, seems quite sure. From the above discussion it seems plain that in reach- ing a numerical measure of the degree of inbreeding it is not sufficient to consider coefficients of inbreeding alone. The coefficients of relationship must also be taken into account. It is suggested that the two constants be written to- gether for each generation, the coefficient of inbreeding being followed by the coefficient of relationship in brackets. Thus we have ga of course, all of a sister’s ancestors are identical with her brother’ tU. s. Dak Agr. Bur. Plant Industry, Bul. No. 243, pp. 1-58, 1912. 522 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vot. XLVIII INBREEDING AND RELATIONSHIP COEFFICIENTS OF Kina MELIA RIOTER 14TH Zy (K,) = 0 (0) Z, (Ky) = 25 (0) Z, (K ) = 25.00 (50.00) Za (K, ) = 37.50 (62.50) Z, (K, ) == 50.00 (75.00) Zs (Ky) ==71.88 (87.50) Z. (K, ) = 81.25 (92.19) Z, (K, ) =90.63 (92.97) Z, (Ky) = 92.77 (93.75) Zə (Ky) = 93.65 (93.75) Zy(Ky) = 93.85 (93.75) Zy,(K,2) = 93.85 (93.75) The physical meaning of these expressions is simple and straightforward. Z,(K,) tells us that in the 5th an- cestral generation of King Melia Rioter 14th he had only one half as many different ancestors as was possible for that generation, and of his ancestors three fourths were common to his sire and his dam. However one looks at .the matter there can be no denial that King Melia Rioter 14th is a closely inbred animal. In Fig. 1 the heavy broken line gives the relationship coefficients for King Melia Rioter 14th. It will be instruct- ive now to consider another example by way of contrast. Again a Jersey bull, Blossom’s Glorene (102701), will be taken. Only the final result need be given. INBREEDING AND RELATIONSHIP COEFFICIENTS OF BLOSSOM’S GLORENE Fel hy) =O (0) ZAKS) y (0) Z.(K,) 12.50 (0) ZE) == 1256 (6) Z,( Ks) = 25.00 (0) Z,(Ke) == 29.69 (0) Ze(K:) = 35.94. (0) Z,(K,) = 40.23 (0) The total inbreeding and the relationship curves are given in Fig. 2. The difference in the breeding of this bull and the one considered in the former example is striking. In the 8th ancestral generation Blossom’s Glorene has but 60 per No. 573] STUDIES ON INBREEDING 523 cent. of the number of different ancestors possible in that generation, but not one single animal in the ancestry of his sire occurs in the ancestry of his dam (within the limits A, to A,). The probability is that Blossom’s Glo- rene is heterozygous in respect of most of his characters, while King Melia Rioter 14th is homozygous. —— —_—— $ N è COEFFICIENTS & i a Se Sa N ae Tee maa i s 4 é 8 40 te ta GENERATIONS Fig, 2. Diagram showing the total inbreeding (heavy solid line) and the relationship (heavy broken line) curves for the Jersey bull Blossom’s Glorene, a period of eight ancestral generations, Compare with Fig. SuMMARY The object of this paper is to call attention to the fact that inbreeding of considerable degree may exist in the entire absence of any kinship between the two individuals bred together, and to bring forward a method of sepa- rately measuring what proportion of the observed in- breeding in a particular case is due to kinship of the pa- rents, and what to earlier ancestral reduplication. A pro- posed coefficient of relationship is described, and its ap- plication illustrated by concrete cases. THE CHROMOSOME HYPOTHESIS OF LINKAGE APPLIED TO CASES IN SWEET PEAS AND PRIMULA From the Zoological Laboratory, Columbia University. CALVIN B. BRIDGES THERE are two views as to the nature of linkage. The earlier view, developed by Bateson and his co-workers, is that this phenomenon is an expression of symmetrical reduplications in the germ tract. A more recent view, developed by Morgan and his co-workers, treats linkage on the basis of a linear arrangement of genes in the chromosomes and of the history of these genes during normal gametogenesis. The advocates of the reduplica- tion view have rarely applied their principles to the re- sults on Drosophila on the ground that the results for Drosophila are complicated by sex-linkage. That sex- linkage is simply an additional, but wholly independent, phenomenon, is proven by the many cases in Drosophila in which sex-linkage is not involved, yet in which the link- age of the genes to each other is of the same type as the linkage of sex-linked genes to each other. In this paper I shall attempt to show. that the theory of linkage which we have successfully applied to all cases in Drosophila, whether involving sex-linked genes or genes which show no sex-linkage, applies equally well to the non-sex-linked cases occurring in sweet peas and primula. The only serious drawback to such an application lies in the nature of the data which have been collected for these eases. The least satisfactory form of data from which to determine a linkage value is that presented by F, results. In eases in which two recessives enter from opposite par- ents (‘‘repulsion’’), the excessive smallness of the double recessive classin F, renders any calculation subject to great error. Slightly better are the F, results from coupling, 524 No. 573] CHROMOSOME HYPOTHESIS OF LINKAGE 525 but here there is no direct parallelism between the zygotic and gametic ratios. In determining what gametic ratio underlies the F, results given by an experiment, the practise has been to compare by the eye the given result with a series of F, results calculated from selected gametic ratios. Collins has shown’ that this practise has led to serious error. In F, coupling cases in which there has been no crossing over in one sex (autosome genes in Drosophila), there is a direct relation between the gametic ` and zygotic series, but only in certain classes which com- prise from one fourth to less than one half of the indi- viduals of an experiment. While such data are more accurate than the usual F, results, yet the percentage of individuals which can be used directly is so low that we avoid the use of such a method. In F, results involving only sex-linked genes, the efficiency is at least 50 per cent., for here there is always a direct relation between the gametic and zygotic ratios in one half the flies (the males). However, half the total number of flies (the females) are useless unless the cross is made in such a way that F, becomes a back cross. These different kinds of F, results (the two most advantageous of which are not generally applicable) are separated in effectiveness by a wide gap from the back cross which we use equally well in all cases, which gives a zygotic ratio directly proportional to the gametic ratio, and in which every individual occurs in the most advantageous relations. Perhaps the least unsatisfactory method of dealing with such F, series as are available in the case of the sweet peas, is by means of the coefficient of association as derived by Yule. Yule’s coefficient of association is caleu- lated from a zygotic series of the form AB:aB: Ab: ab by the formula: Coefficient of association = 4B corer on = i To find the gametic ratio corresponding to this coeffi- cient, use is made of a table which gives the coefficients 1 Am. Nart., ’12. 526 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST — [Vou. XLVIII calculated from the zygotic series corresponding to such gametic ratios as 2.5:1, 3:1, 3.5:1, ete. For the same ratio in the coupling and repulsion series the coefficients are slightly different, so that two tables should be made. -Upon the chromosome basis the best method of express- ing the amount of linkage is in terms of percentage of crossing over. The gametie ratio n:1 found through the coefficient Di association, when expressed as a percentage becomes - ot i. According to the chromosome hypothesis, all genes which are linked to each other lie in the same chromosome. In sweet peas the first case in which linkage was observed was that of round pollen? and red flower color. Later it was found that hooded standard was linked to round and to red. The genes for these three characters, then, may be treated as though carried by the same chromosome, which we may call chromosome I, of the sweet pea. The relative distances of these genes from one another in the chromosome can be determined from the degrees of linkage. The farther apart in the chromosome any two genes lie, the greater will be the amount of crossing over between them. If two genes lie very close together, then. the percentage of crossing-over will be very small (the gametic ratio very large). Fortunately Punnett has recently collected the data upon these linkage cases in sweet peas. In the table which follows, I have summarized the data given by the various tables of Punnett. In the first column to the right of the data appear the coefficients of association. In the next column appear the corresponding gametic ratios caleu- lated by interpolation to the nearest tenth. In the last column are the equivalent percentages of crossing over, found from the gametic ratios. We may use one per cent. of crossing over as our unit of distance in measuring the space between two genes. 2 T have used a terminology here like that used for the cases in Drosophila, naming the gene after that member of the pair of allelomorphs which may be considered as the mutant from the wild type of pea. No. 573] CHROMOSOME HYPOTHESIS OF LINKAGE 527 The gene for red is then about eleven units from that for round, and the gene for hooded is nearly one unit from that for red. TABLE I CHROMOSOME I Round Pollen and Red Color Coefficient Percent- of age of Associa- Gametic Cross- Wild Type Round Red Round Red tion Ratio overs Coupling shed OT 583 614 2,197 9596 7.931 11% Red Color and Hooded Standard Wild Type Red Hooded Red Hooded Coupling ...2,568 16 17 857 9998 125023 38 Round Pollen and Hooded Standard Wild Type Round Hooded Round Hooded Coupling ... 626 74 83 174 .8932 4.7:1 18. Repulsion .,.3,140 1,413 1,438 14 9577 SASL 10.3 The order of arrangement of these genes in the chromo- some can be discovered from a comparison of the linkage values found above: The linkage value (11.2) for round and red is the most accurately determined of those in- volved, so that we may lay this down às our initial or base line: Ro R 0 11.2 DIAGRAM I. Rọ= round pollen, R=red flower. The next most accurate value is that for red and hooded, namely, 0.8. Hooded lies therefore only about one unit from red, but if these two values only, namely, round red and red hooded, were given, we should be un- able to decide whether hooded lies between round and red at a position near 10 (that is, 11.2 — .8) or beyond red in a locus at 12 (that is, 11.2+.8). In order to determine whether hooded lies to the left or to the right of red the data for the third value, round hooded, need only be accu- rate enough for us to decide between these values of 10 528 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII and of 12 units. The data from the coupling experiments (which even though less extensive then those from the repulsion experiments are probably more accurate) give a value of about 18 units. Since the repulsion data give 10 units, 18 is probably too high, and an intermediate position correct. The higher (12) of the two possible values is then the correct value. The position at 10 is not excluded by these data, but is far less probable. In a case in which one of the two first values is very small, as here, the accuracy demanded of the remaining or third value is much greater than in cases where neither of the values are small, and one has only to decide between two very different values by aid of the third. There are other ways of arriving at this order of genes which are independent of the size of the values. One of those methods, such for example, as that of double crossing over, would definitely settle the order of these three genes, but unfortunately such data have not yet been published. If hooded lies beyond red at 12, the complete first chro- mosome diagram will be as follows: Ro RH 0 2 2. DiaGrAM II. Chromosome I, Sweet Pea. Rọ= round pollen, R= red flower, H ooded, In the above diagram R, indicates the locus of round (and also of long). The symbols in the diagrams are used to designate loci which may be occupied by either allelomorph of the pair. It has been observed that hooded flowers have always a uniform color in standard and wings, instead of having these two regions colored differently as in the normal or bicolor type. Bateson assumed that this unicolorism was only another somatic effect of the hooded gene. However, an alternative explanation is that the unicolor is caused by a specific gene which is very closely linked to hooded. If this should be found to be the case, then this fourth gene also will be located at about 12 units from round. No. 573] CHROMOSOME HYPOTHESIS OF LINKAGE 529 There is one other gene which probably belongs in the first chromosome, namely, the intensifier found in the ‘ black knight’’ race. The linkage data of red color and intensity of color have been given in Report II to the Evolution Committee, page 90. TABLE II Red Color and Intense Color ~~ Feoi o Associs- Gametic Cross- Wild Type Red Intense Red Intense tion Ratio overs Coupling eee |] 29 35 22 527 49:1 If these data are significant, then intense is in the first chromosome at a locus about 35 to the right or left of red. It should give about 24 (35 — 11) or 46 (35 + 11) per cent. of crossing over with round, depending on whether it lies about 24 to the left of round or 35 to the right of red. THE SECOND CHROMOSOME OF SWEET PEAS In the case of the second chromosome in sweet peas, the linkage values are based on smaller numbers, but the order of genes is more certain. The first linkage case of this imon was that of sterile anthers and light axils. Later the cretin form of ower was found to belong to this linkage group. As in the case of the first chromosome, I have summarized the tables of Punnett in Table III. TABLE III CHROMOSOME II Sterile Anthers and Light Axil Coefficient Percent- of ; Associs- Gametic Cross- a d Type Sterile Light Sterile Light tion Ratio overs Cansine p 170 41 30 379 .9945 22.: 1 4.4 Repulsion . 1,335 643 714 2 -988 20.:1 4.9 Light Axils and Cretin Flower Wild Type Light Cretin Light Cretin Coupling . . 282 49 52 59 .734 2.6:1 28. Ropulkiok - so 22 27 3 .610 27:1 27. 530 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII Sterile Anthers and Cretin Flower Wild Type Sterile Cretin Sterile Cretin Coupling mera! Ls 58 78 55 21 33. Repulsion .. 764 355 345 25 .683 2.6:1 28. The linkage value for sterile and light, namely, 4.4 units, ‘is the most accurately determined of those in the second chromosome. The value for light and cretin is about 28 units. Using the distance 4.4 between sterile and light as our base line, then, we should find that cretin lies at 4 + 28 or 32 from sterile if the order of genes is sterile, light, cretin; but if the order is cretin, sterile, light, then cretin should lie at 28 — 4 or 24 from sterile. The value for sterile cretin should approximate either 24 or 32. There is no very small value here as there was in the first chro- mosome, and not such great accuracy is required of the remaining value, since it should be easy to distinguish be- tween 24 and 32. The coupling data for this value gives 33 units, which enables us to fix the order of genes as sterile, light, cretin. The following diagram of chromo- some IT expresses these relations more clearly. L C 4.4 32. D1aGRaAM III. Chromosome II, Sweat Pea. S= sterile, L = light, C = cretin. orm When crossing over is as free as in the case of sterile and cretin and of light and cretin there should be some double crossing over. That is, crossing over might occur in the section of the chromosome near sterile and light and at the same time another crossover could occur in the section between light and cretin. This occurrence would be readily seen if normal plants heterozygous in any com- bination of these three genes were back-crossed to plants purely recessive in all three. A relatively few plants from such a test would give very valuable information on sev- eral points, while an experiment of a few thousand indi- viduals from such back-cross tests would enable one to discover, through the phenomenon of interference, much No. 573] CHROMOSOME HYPOTHESIS OF LINKAGE 531 as to the character of the chromosome, the average length of the internode, and the percentage of chiasmas per node. INDEPENDENCE OF CHROMOSOMES I AND II or Sweet Pras If two groups of genes are carried by separate chromo- somes, we may expect to obtain free assortment and typical 9:3:3:1 ratios in F,, when any two genes from different groups are involved. There are rather extensive data for three such cases in sweet peas, and in each there is practically complete independence. The data given in able IV are summarized from Report III to the Evolu- tion Committee (page 37) and Report IV (page 17). TABLE IV INDEPENDENCE OF THE FIRST AND SECOND CHROMOSOMES Round Pollen (1st) and Light Axil (2d) : wes apne Percent- ; Associ Wild Type Round Light Round Light tion 1,246 341 399 142 ASL 225001 47. age a- Gametie Cross- Ratio, Ts Red Color (1st) and Light Axil (2d) Wild Type Red Light Red Light 1,563 545 506 232 .136 LI6:1 47. Red Color (1st) and Sterile Anthers (2d) Wild Type Red Sterile Red Sterile 838 403 265 071 10i B The greatest departure from the 50 per cent. of cross- ing over expected from independent assortment is only to 47 per cent. There are several other characters whose genes seem to be independent of those in the first and second chromo- somes. This is interesting from the point of view that each independent gene or group of linked genes requires a distinct chromosome as a carrier. 508 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST — [Vou. XLVII LinKAGE CASES IN PRIMULA In the case of primula, linkage was first found between red (versus green) stigma and red (versus magenta) flower color. Long style (versus short) and dark stem (versus light) were found to be linked with red stigma. Indications were observed that still a fifth gene, a domi- nant which reduces the color of the flower to a tinge in the corolla tube, belonged to this group. back cross involving the three genes, red stigma, red flower and long style was made. Credit is due to Gregory for the use of this method for obtaining linkage data. Unfortunately many of the individuals were useless for the linkage of red flower color, because of the occurrence of white; and the numbers are small. In Table V, I have summarized the data given by Gregory.* TABLE V THE First CHROMOSOME OF PRIMULA Red Stigma and Red Flower Non-crossovers Crossovers ah Q z vo ES 2 àc i Bo wf ei yi ees zs EB od Fe ag mS egs ge 295 2a Os 7 a” Coupling DOCK eros oiio 28 39 17 18 1.9:1 34.6 Wild Red Red Red Stigma Type Stigma Flower Red Flower Coupling Fes... 1,174 305 289 232 O11 1.8:1 35.3 Red Flower and Long Style Non-crossovers Crossovers Red Long Wild Type Red Long Coupling back cross. 40 53 6 5 8.4:1 10.9 Wild Type Red Long Red Long Coupling fT ie oar 38 2 n o 12 .966 8.6:1 10.4 Red Stigma and Long, Style n Coupling back cross. 44 64 5 30 1.6:1 37. 4 Jour. Genetics, ’11, Vol. I; Proe. Roy. Soc., ’11, Vol. —, 84. No. 573] CHROMOSOME HYPOTHESIS OF LINKAGE 533 Red Stigma and Dark Stem Wild Type Red Stigma Dark Red Stigma Dark mepulsion 34255 k 137 66 62 0 -— — — The three values are—red stigma red flower 35, red flower long style 11, and red stigma long style 37. Of these, red stigma red flower is based upon the most data, _ and may therefore be taken as our base line. The value for red stigma long style should be 35 — 11 or 24, if the order of genes is long, red stigma, red flower; but 35 + 11 or 46, if the order of genes is red stigma, red flower, long. The value shown by the table is 37. This means that long lies to the right of red at a locus 46. R; R L 0: 35. 46. DIAGRAM IV. Chromosome I, Primula. Rs= red stigma, R= red flower, L = long t style. 58 59 > 60 61 62 The apparent discrepancy between the values 46 and 37 is due in most part to double crossing over, the effect of which is always to lower large values disproportion- ately more than short. When the discrepancy is known, the amount of double crossing over can be calculated approximately. Here the amount of double crossing over is 46 — 37 a That is, 4.5 per cent. of all the gametes are the result of double crossing over. A somewhat larger amount of data from a back cross in which all the individuals are effective would give by direct experiment a true value for the amount of double crossing over. A chromosome diagram should be built up of values independent of double crossing over. According to our experience with Drosophila, if there is not more than ten per cent. of crossing over between two genes, the double crossing over is negligible. Thus in the first chromosome in sweet peas, the values obtained from the experiments are not changed by double crossing over. However, in the 534 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII case of the-second chromosome, where the total percentage of crossing over is about 32, there is probably one or two per cent. of double crossing over. The diagram of the second chromosome is in this respect only tentative, and the plotted position of cretin will be moved a little farther to the right when the amount of double crossing over between light and cretin has been found. The value 4.4 for sterile anther light axil is not affected by double crossing over, since the section of chromosome between these two loci is so short that a double break would prob- ably not occur between them at all. The amount of double crossing over between any two loci can only be found when there is a gene between them. Thus if a gene should be found which lies between light and cretin, either by indirect calculation or, better, by direct experiment, the amount of double crossing over could be found. The more genes which can be worked with in the same chromo- some, the more accurate becomes the diagram. All the values found for these cases in sweet peas and primula are based upon such small numbers that they can be used only as illustrations of the way in which one would apply to new cases certain principles worked out in Drosophila. While they serve as examples in line with these principles, they are entirely inadequate as proof. A very interesting case of variation in linkage is pre- sented by some of the families involving chromosome IT of the sweet pea. In this article I have avoided such data as far as I could, but it is possible that the order in which I have aligned these genes will be found to be incorrect when data upon all three genes in a back cross are ob- tained. Such data would show, through the phenomenon of double crossing over, what the order of genes is, even though variations in the linkage should occur. CoLUMBIA UNIVERSITY, May, 1914 THE REDUPLICATION HYPOTHESIS AS APPLIED TO DROSOPHILA Dr. A. H. STURTEVANT COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY A NUMBER of papers developing the reduplication hy- pothesis of linkage have recently appeared in the Journal of Genetics. They are based almost entirely on the experiments of Gregory (’11) on Primula and of Punnett (713) on the sweet pea. The data are not entirely satis- factory because of the relatively small number of genes involved, and because in most cases the gametic ratios can be only approximately determined. This is due to the fact that most of the data concern F, counts, from which gametic ratios can not be calculated directly. In Gregory’s best case a much more satisfactory method was followed— the heterozygous plants were tested, not by mating to others of their kind, but by crossing with plants recessive with respect to all the genes involved, which gives the gametic ratio directly. In this case, however, we have only a relatively small series of data involving as many as three pairs of linked genes. It is obvious that from such data no adequate test of the reduplication hypothesis can be made. The phenomena of linkage have been very extensively studied, by Morgan and others, in the fly Drosophila. In this animal there are many genes belonging to the same linkage groups, and these have been studied on a large scale. In the case of the sex-linked group there is never any difficulty in calculating the gametice ratio from F, results, since the F, males from any cross always give it directly. I have recently published a paper (Sturtevant, 14) giving a complete summary of the published results obtained from studies of the linkage of these genes. In that paper I have adopted the chromosome explanation of link- 535 536 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vow XLVIII age proposed by Morgan (711). Here I shall use the same data for a test of the reduplication theory. It may be of value to contrast the two views by making a rigorous application of them to the same facts. Since the data concerning the sex-linked group of genes in Drosophila form the simplest and most extensive series now avail- able, I shall deal more especially with them. The reader is referred to my other paper for the detailed data, for references to original sources, and for a full treatment of the chromosome hypothesis as applied to these and other data. It may be well to give first a brief catalogue of the sex-linked genes discussed in this paper. The nomen- clature is that suggested by Morgan (713). This may be confusing to those accustomed to the ‘‘presence and ab- sence’’ system, but this should not be a serious objection here, since a clear conception of the somatic appearance of the animals discussed is not essential for our present purpose. The relations would be as clear if hieroglyphics were used for symbols. Y is the gene which differentiates the wild ‘‘gray’’ bodied fly from the yellow mutant, y. V differentiates the wild red-eyed fly from the ver- milion-eyed mutant, v. : M differentiates the ‘‘long’’ wing of the wild fly from that of the miniature-winged mutant, m. R is another gene affecting the wings. The wild fly has R, the rudimentary-winged mutant has r. Br’ occurs in a dominant mutant form having a narrow eye known as barred. The allelomorph present in the wild fly is designated br’. The other characters concerned bear such a relation to one another that the genes involved are considered as forming a system of quadruple allelomorphs. The alter- native to this view is the assumption of complete linkage, but I have given elsewhere (Sturtevant, ’13) my reasons for preferring the multiple allelomorph interpretation. The eye of the wild Drosophila is red in color. A single No. 573] REDUPLICATION HYPOTHESIS 537 mutant obtained from it had white eyes (Morgan, 710), and this character proved to be a simple sex-linked reces- sive. From the white-eyed form arose a fly with eosin eyes (Morgan, ’12). This new character was found to be a sex-linked dominant to white, and a sex-linked recessive to red. Finally, there arose a form with cherry eye color (Safir, 713). This has the same relation to red and to white as has eosin. Mated to eosin it gives an inter- mediate color, which splits up into cherry, intermediate, and eosin in F,. The nomenclature adopted in this case is as follows: Allelomorph present in the red-eyed fly, W. Allelomorph present in the white-eyed fly, w. Allelomorph present in the eosin-eyed fly, w°. Allelomorph present in the cherry-eyed fly, w°. Trow (713) has suggested the possibility of an asym- metrical reduplication series, giving a gametic series of WAB:xAb:yaB:zab, where w need not equal z, nor x equal y. It should be noted that an actual demonstra- tion of such a ratio, or of its non-existence, is almost ex- cluded for the reason that it would be practically impos- sible to be sure one was not dealing with a case involving differential viability. However, perhaps the most stri- king general fact brought out by the study of linkage is that each pair of linked genes (allelomorphs), considered separately, follows a perfectly regular Mendelian course. I think we are, therefore, justified in assuming that the number of gametes bearing A is always equal to the num- ber bearing a, and similarly for B and b. Then, in Trow’s asymmetrical series, wtae=yte, w+ y=sr +z. Hence, w=2 and t= y. In all that follows I shall assume that the reduplication series are always symmetrical. On this assumption it becomes unnecessary to consider the two halves of the 538 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII series separately, and I shall therefore use only two terms in speaking of gametic ratios. By adding together the two halves of the series larger numbers are obtained, so that chance deviations are relatively smaller. Differential viability is also partially overcome in this way. Of course on the reduplication theory both terms of the gametic ratio must be integers, since they represent num- bers of cells, but nevertheless it has seemed to me more convenient for purposes of calculation to express them always in the form n:1. Thus a gametic ration of 3:2 may be written 1.5:1. It was suggested by Bateson and Punnett (711) that the intensity of coupling and of repulsion between the same two pairs of genes may be identical. That this is substantially the case has been shown again and again in Drosophila, and has become a truism among those work- ing on that form. Before presenting data on this point I wish to bring up another matter on which the same data have a bearing. Punnett (’13) has said, ‘‘ But where three [pairs of] factors are concerned . . . the value of the primary reduplications is evidently altered, and there would seem to be some process whereby these reduplica- tions react on one another.’’ Bailey (’14) has suggested that the nature of this interaction may be such as to cause the two primary series to be of equal intensity. It may be categorically stated that there is no interaction effect in Drosophila. The best data for a test of the relative inten- sity of coupling and repulsion, and of ‘‘fundamental,’’ ‘‘primary’’ and ‘‘secondary’’ reduplication series, in- volving the same allelomorphic groups, is that furnished by the relations of the various forms of W (W, w, w°, w°) to the M pair of allelomorphs (M and m). Table I is a summary of the data on this case. In computing the fundamental series I have used only the data from such of my own experiments as involve only two pairs of genes, since that from other sources is for the most part made up of primary series in which the other primary series in- volved is masked. No. 573] _ REDUPLICATION HYPOTHESIS 539 TABLE I FUNDAMENTAL SERIES Nature of Cross Actual Numbers Gametie Ratios WM X wm 16+:1 Wm X wM 93: 221 1: 2.4 — WM X wem 634: 348 1.8 +: 1 Wm X weM n Ge 120 1: 2.4 — Wm X weM 46l: 855 1: 1.9 — weM X wm . 4,171: 1,858 2.2 +:1 wem X wM 891: 1,898 1: 2.1 + weM X wm woe 47 Fe: t PRIMARY SERIES ; Other Primary Nature of Cross Actual Numbers Gametic Ratio Series Involved 85 2.1—:1 MBr wem X wM 69: -122 1:13 — MBr’ WM X wm 5,838: 2,911 2.0 +:1 YW Wm X wM 1,111: 2,493 1: 2.2 + YW WM X wm 2,261: 1,011 2.2 +:1 MR Secondary Series Primary Series WM X wem so) PIO: 407 1.8—:1 WF, VM Wm X weM 227: 509 l: 2.2 — WV, VM It will be noted that in all these cases the gametic ratio approximates 2:1, or 1:2, according to the nature of the cross. There are only four cases showing a noticeable deviation from this value, and of these two involve only small counts. The most serious is the first. In this case there is a deviation of 54.3 from the 2:1 ratio, and the stand- ard error is 16.7[V1/3 x 2/3 X (777 + 470) = + 16.7-]. Since the deviation is slightly over three times the stand- ard error, it is perhaps significant, especially since there is at least one other rather large deviation (the second ratio in Table I). For our present purpose, however, it is probably not significant, since similar deviations occur in different experiments of exactly the same type. I have recorded elsewhere (Sturtevant, ’14) the results of a num- ber of tests of individual females heterozygous for these two allelomorphic groups. Taking only those cultures which produced 100 or more flies, we find the following results: | 540 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII Seven females of the constitution w°mwM gave gametic ratios ranging from 1.5:1 to 2.7:1, with the modal class at about 2.0:1. Seventeen females w°Mwm gave ratios ranging from 1.5:1 to 3.4:1, with a single individual at 4.2:1. The modal class was at about 2.2:1. - It seems highly probable that all these deviations from a 2:1 ratio, not due to insufficient numbers, may be satis- factorily explained on the basis of differential viability, which is known to occur here (for a discussion of the vagaries of differential viability see Bridges and Sturte- vant, 14). I do not wish to be understood as arguing that the gametic ratio for any two pairs of genes is abso- lutely constant, but only that it is in most cases uninflu- enced by the way in which the genes are combined and by heterozygosis for other genes. That it may sometimes show marked differences is now well established. I have myself studied two eases of this sort, and I have good evidence (not yet published in detail) that there are defi- nite genes which cause great differences in the gametic ratios for whole linkage groups. In one case this gene itself shows linkage to those in the group it affects. But even here the intensity of coupling and of repulsion is affected alike, and it makes no difference how few or how many genes a fly is heterozygous for; the linkage is strong or weak according to the form of the linkage-affecting gene which the fly happens to carry. In each of these cases I have been able to obtain about the same extreme values both for coupling and for repulsion. In what follows I shall assume that the intensity of the reduplication series is not affected by the way in which the genes are introduced, nor by the number of linked genes involved in the cross. The obvious corollary of this is that reduplication occurs even in homozygous indi- viduals, and that the nature of the series of divisions is in general independent of the constitution of the indi- vidual. This conclusion is directly opposed to the point of view expressed more especially by Punnett, in the No. 573] REDUPLICATION HYPOTHESIS 541 passage quoted above and elsewhere. If reduplication occurs at all it is the same in the wild fly as in the most complex linkage experiment we have yet carried out. If it is assumed that the intensity of coupling and re- pulsion is identical, it becomes unnecessary to consider them separately. I shall therefore lump together all the data involving the same groups of allelomorphs, regard- less of how they were put into the cross. When three pairs of genes are involved there are eight possible com- binations of them in F., but only four if we add together the two halves of the reduplication diagram. There are the two original combinations, which I shall designate ABC. Then there are three combinations derived from each of these by a shifting of one gene, which I shall designate ABc, AbC and aBC, the small letters referring to those pairs which have been shifted. Thus, to take an imaginary case, if we cross LMn by lmN, the gametes produced by the F, individuals will be classified as follows: ABC ABc AbC aBC LMn LMN Lmn lMn ImN lmn IMN LmN In the following tables I shall reduce all data to this form. In each case the genes will be arranged so that AB and BC will be the primary reduplication series. Table II contains such a summary of all the crosses in- volving three pairs of sex-linked genes. Table III shows the gametie ratios derived from these data, and also the values for the secondary series calculated on the basis of Trow’s ‘‘special’’ hypothesis. For the sake of brevity only one term is used: a gametic ratio of 3:1 is written 3; a ratio of 3:2 becomes 1.5, ete. With the simplifications introduced here Trow’s formula becomes ac — (48 x BO) +1 AP Eee. 1 As was pointed out by Punnett (’13), in a system of three reduplica- tion series the one with the lowest intensity is to be regarded as the second- 542 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVII TABLE II Allelomorphic Groups ABC ABc | AbC aBC BBO) Cp PE ORT CATES Se Sar 8,212 4,013 9 119 BOW fe i a ea oe 278 60 0 PVM SEU Ree 1,082 58 y 22 665 PE kee a SA 315 138 55 196 TIEF Sele Pee eS 93 34 10 54 WEM, ie ais 194 1 102 PUER ee. ee eet 1,726 535 139 872 EMED ee eee 20 73 25 129 TABLE III Gametic ratios Experiment Observed Calculated AB BC AC AC > A” Sree ee 6 as 95.5 2.0— 2.0+ FNE oe ee oe 438.0 1.74 1.72 1.74 VY Mero t.7 22.0 1b 1.6 FFR abet SiMe Segall 1.8 gE 1.3 PV Bi ee oc rae 2.0 3.4 1.3 1.4 AES ee eo 2.0 24.7 LZ 1.9 FNE eee 2.9 ; 1.3 1.6 FE ace oe ee ee 1.9 3.6 T2 1.4 It will be seen that in every case the calculated value for the secondary reduplication is higher than the ob- served value. The same relation comes out in two experi- ments which I have done involving genes of another group in Drosophila (see Table VIII, Sturtevant, ’14). Punnett’s case is so involved that calculations accurate enough for our present purpose can not be made. In Gregory’s experiment one of the genes (M) could not be followed in all the plants because masked by another gene. We are not given the data for S and G in those plants in which M was classified separately from those in which it was not. The data are therefore not available for exact calculations, since the numbers are too small to overcome chance deviations. The data for my own two experi- ments appear in Table IV. The same relation comes out more strikingly in another way. If we let m equal the intensity of the AB series and n that of the BC series, then on Trow’s special hypothesis No. 573] REDUPLICATION HYPOTHESIS 543 the four kinds of gametes should occur in the following proportions: ABC — mn ABc—m aBC'—n AbC —1 TABLE IV Observed Calculated Experiment AB cll | AC AC Big rey 3.4 11.6 | 2.4 2.7 CCR E N A 2.5 2:1 1.0 1.4 That is, 1/(m + 1) of the gametes should have A and B interchanged. Of these, 1/(n +1) should have B and C also interchanged. If N represents the total number of gametes, then the size of the AbC class should be repre- sented by the expression AbC = N (m+ 1)(n+1)° Table V shows the relation between the size of this class as observed and as thus calculated, in the ten experiments. Allelomorphiec or eee eee TABLE V ABC “Observed Calculated Era Ce 9 42 ERTE 0 0 pee eee a 22 30 FRN E 55 69 R E E 10 15 rem er l 4 E Rease 139 208 PAE A A 25 34 AA E 2 7 Dic ce a 12 20 Thus it appears that in all ten experiments Trow’s formula gives values for the AC series and for the AbC 544 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII term which are too large. Moreover, this feature appears in a more complex cross which I have carried out, in- volving four pairs of linked genes (YWV WM), and in each separate part of all these experiments, regardless of how the crosses were made. It may, then, be taken as a con- stant relation. It can only mean that there is some rela- tion between A and C besides that resulting from second- ary reduplication. In other words, to use Bailey’s terms, Trow’s ‘‘special’’ hypothesis is not valid. Let us then examine what Bailey calls Trow’s ‘‘gen- eral” hypothesis. Suppose the primary series to be of the following values: AB ==1:1, BO een it, AC 2 nz i. Trow’s general formula for calculating what should be the observed value of-the AC series is The special formula is derived from this by assuming n = 1, when the formula becomes Be nae: pgm. Since this always gives a value which is too large, it follows that n is always less than one. This means that the AC primary series is reversed—that the combinations present in the parents tend to be reproduced in fewer numbers than the new combinations. I have worked this out for the case of BCvSp (see Table IV), and find the primary series there to be 0.6:1, though the observed series is 1.0. The ‘“‘fundamental”’ AC series has been . obtained for most of the eases in Table III, and has always been found to be of the usual form (i. e., n:1, AC No. 573] REDUPLICATION HYPOTHESIS 545 where n >1). (See Table I, Sturtevant, ’14.) In fact, as stated above, the fundamental series always approxi- mates the secondary (observed) series. There are two hypotheses as to the mechanics of re- duplication series where more than two pairs of genes are involved. The first was suggested by Bateson and Pun- nett (711), and consists in the assumption that when three pairs are involved eight cells are formed by three succes- sive divisions, each of which segregates one pair of genes. The eight cells then represent the eight possible kinds of gametes, and are supposed to reduplicate independently until the proper proportions are reached. Bailey sup- poses that if it be shown that two primary series do not interact on each other this scheme will be more likely to be correct than will Trow’s, which I shall discuss next. It seems to me, however, that this hypothesis begs the ques- tion. It is derived entirely by working backwards from the observed results; it affords no basis for predictions; and it does not offer a simple mechanical explanation of any of the observed results. For pragmatic reasons I believe we should adopt it only as a last resort. - Trow supposes that two cell divisions occur, segregating two pairs of genes. The four resulting cells then go through with their reduplication, which is a primary one. When this is finished there occur divisions which segre- gate the other pair, and the other primary reduplication is carried out. On Trow’s general hypothesis, which I have tried to show is the only one which can hold, it is supposed that the second series of reduplications is affected by both of the first two pairs of genes. C is re- duplicating more if with B than if with b, less if with A than if with a. This scheme of Trow’s has one great advantage in that it accounts for the fact that the class which I have called AbC is always the smallest one. Reference to Trow’s calculations will show that this rela- tion should always occur, and Table IT shows that it does occur. .On the octant scheme there is no explanation of this relation—we oie have to assume that it does occur somehow. 546 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST. [Vou XLVIII It will be noted that several of the gametic ratios in- volved here closely approach 2:1. YV, YM, WV and WM are the most conspicuous examples. It may seem that such a simple ratio is due to a very simple reduplication series, but I do not think such an assumption can be suc- cessfully maintained. The tables given above show that YM and WM have approximately this same value when they appear as secondary series, and the data for the combination YWVM show the same thing for YV (see Sturtevant, 714). If, as I have maintained above, the same series of redu- plications must occur in all flies, whether we can follow it or not, then it follows that in these three cases the 2:1 ratio is never due to a simple series, but always to a long and complicated one, since in all three one of the primary series is of high intensity. It was pointed out by Trow that the intensities of the reduplication series afford a method of calculating the number of cell divisions necessary to complete the series. If we assume that approximately the same series is occurring both in homozygous and in heterozygous flies, we have the following series in Drosophila as a basis for such calculations. Sex-linked Group EW 901 Wy =: 24 VM== 318 MR =: 30 RB x217 Second Group BVg= 3.6 VgCv= 104 CuSp= 2.8 SpBa= 10+ Third Group PEb=100+ No. 573] REDUPLICATION HYPOTHESIS 547 All of these series must be considered as either primary or secondary and therefore involving primaries of higher intensity. In fact there is unpublished evidence that many of them can not be simple primaries. A num- ber of series of very high intensity are known, and will appear in future publications. Therefore all the calcula- tions that follow give results which are far too small. According to Trow, the minimal number of successive cell divisions required to complete the series is given by the expression mnp --- where m, n, p, ete., are the larger terms of the primary series involved. In the present case the value of that expression is something over 76,000,- 000,000. However, Trow’s formula seems to be wrong. If a be the number of cell divisions required to produce m cells, then 2*—m. If this expression gives a value of a which is not an integer, then the next higher whole number is to be taken. In the case of the first series two divisions are necessary to segregate the genes, and in the following series one is required. The number of succes- sive cell divisions required then is (a+1)+(b+1) +(e+1)+----+1, where b, c, ete., bear the same rela- tion to n, p, etc., that a does to m. In the case of Droso- phila the value of this expression is 56. As pointed out, however, this value is certainly far too small. The total number of cells required is given by the aches a ee t d --- t2mn--- + 2mp + 2m - r -+ 2p: +--+ +2mnp + 2mn + 2mp Eat ete REWI This gives a value considerably above 600,000,000,000—a manifest absurdity. However, it is not necessary that all these cells should be produced, since the ratios would not be appreciably affected by some lines becoming crowded out. It is necessary, on the other hand, that all of the series shall be completed in every line which does live, since every female Drosophila, which is of the proper constitution to be tested, shows n for every pair of genes tested. 2 The results discussed here deal only with the linkage in female flies. 548 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII Thus we are forced to assume an enormously complex series of cell divisions, many of them differential, pro- ceeding with mathematical regularity and precision, but in a manner for which direct observation furnishes no basis. It seems to me that it is not desirable to assume such a complex series of events unless we have extremely strong reasons for doing so. I can see no sound reason for adopting the reduplication hypothesis. It apparently rests on two discredited hypotheses: somatic segregation, and the occurrence of members of the 3:1, 7:1, 15:1, etc., series of gametic ratios in more cases than would be ex- pected from a chance distribution. The chief advantage of the chromosome hypothesis of linkage which has been proposed by Morgan (711), an which I have followed elsewhere, seems to me to be its simplicity. In addition it appeals to a known mechanism, and a mechanism toward which the experiments of Boveri, Herbst, Baltzer and others point as the correct one. It explains everything that any of the forms of the redupli- cation hypothesis does, and in addition offers a simple mechanical explanation of the fact that ‘‘secondary series’’ are always smaller than Trow’s ‘‘special hypoth- esis’’ calls for them to be. On the reduplication hypoth- esis this fact must merely be accepted, for, I think, it can not be explained. COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY, LITERATURE CITED Bailey, P. G. 714. Primary and Secondary Reduplication Series. Jour. Genet., II. Bateson, W., and R. C. Punnett. 11, On Gametic Series Involving Reduplication of Certain Terms. Jour. Genet., I. cy a C. B., and A. H. Sturtevant, A New Gene in the Second Chromosome of Drosophila, ete. Biol. Bull., XXVI. Sgor, A E 11. On Gametie Coupling and Repulsion in Primula sinensis. Proc. Royal Soc., 84. B. Morgan, T. H. 710. Sex Limited Inheritance in Drosophila, Science, XXXII. No. 573] REDUPLICATION HYPOTHESIS 549 711. An Attempt to Analyze the Constitution of the Chromosomes on the Basis of Sex-limited Inheritance in Drosophila. Jour. Exp. i 00 712. Further Experiments with Mutations in Eye-color in Drosophila. . Acad. Nat. Sci. Philadelphia 713. Atti and Unit Characters in Mendelian Heredity. AMER. NAT., XLVII. Punnett, R. C. 13. Reduplication Series in Sweet Peas. Jour. Genet., III. Safir, S. R. ’13. A New Eye-color Mutation in Drosophila. Biol. Bull, XXV. Sturtevant, A. ”19: The Bitwlinyan Rabbit Case, with Some neers see on Mul- tiple Allelomorphs. AMER. Nat., XLVII. 714, The Behavior of the Cheaniobniliee as Studied Through Linkage. Zeits. f. ind. Abst.- u. Vererb.-Lehre Trow, A. H. 713. Forms of Reduplication—Primary and Secondary. Jour. Genet., II PATTERN DEVELOPMENT IN MAMMALS AND BIRDS. Ill GLOVER M. ALLEN Boston Museum or NATURAL HISTORY PARTIAL ALBINISM IN Wp BIRDS In birds under natural conditions of wild life partial albinism is fairly common. Lists of species of which - albinistic specimens are known were published by Ruth- ven Deane (1876, 1880) some years ago, and by others. Scattered instances are in all the bird journals or maga- zines of general natural history. In most cases in which the white markings are clearly defined against the pig- mented parts of the plumage, these may be referred to their particuiar primary breaks between the several areas of pigment formation. In other cases the pigment reduction is of the diffuse type, tending to form spots. A few instances follow in which the several primary patches have been observed in wild birds, either as acci- dental marks or as permanent parts of the pattern. The Crown Patch.—In 1908, a pair of robins nested near Lowell Park, Cambridge, one of which showed a partial separation of the crown patch, through the pres- ence of a white band, as broad as the eye’s diameter, passing from one eye around the back of the head to the other eye. In the Wilson Bulletin (Vol. 2, p. 45, 1908) W. E. Saunders records the capture of two robins each with a white collar about the neck, probably marking the separation of the neek patches from the shoulder patches. Coues (1878) records a brood of black robins at St. John’s, N. B., one of which was kept in captivity by the late G. A. Boardman. In September, after moulting, it was still pure black, except for white wings and tail, which seems to indicate an areal restriction of the shoulder and rump patches, though the pigment, where 550 No. 573] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 551 produced, must have been superabundant. Ward (1908) has described a case of a black robin becoming albinistic and reviews a number of such cases. The ability of the same feather follicles in different moults to produce feathers with different sorts or amounts of pigment is thus evidenced and has lately been carefully studied by Pearl and Boring (1914) in the hen. In addition to the case of the robin above mentioned, the white line marking off the crown patch from the ear patches is sometimes found abnormally in other birds. Thus Sweet (1907) records two slate-colored juncos (Junco hyemalis) taken in March, 1903, at Avon, Maine, in which there was a white line above the eye, and the black throat patch was absent, owing no doubt to the ventral restriction of the neck patches, as often seen, for example in pigeons. Maynard! figures the head of a young female black-poll warbler (Dendroica striata) in autumn, showing an inclination to assume a white super- ciliary stripe. I am convinced that this mark so common in many birds, is merely a development of the primary break marking off the crown patch from the ear patches so that it has become a permanent part of the pattern. The failure of the crown patch to develop at all, as is sometimes the case in the domestic pigeon, results in a white-crowned bird. In the West Indian Columba leuco- cephala, exactly this modification has taken place and the entire top of the head is permanently white. The same condition is found in sundry other genera, including a humming bird, a heron, and others. It would be inter- esting to discover by experiment if it were not easier to produce a definite white marking through selecting for the non-development of a certain patch or patches, than to try to restrict a certain pigment patch to definite bounds as in the experiments of Dr. MacCurdy and Pro- fessor Castle (1907). The crown patch as a separate unit in pigmentation, is often of a different hue from the surrounding patches. 1‘‘ Birds of E. North America,’’ 1896, p. 585. 552 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [VoL. XLVIII Thus in the case of the terns, the black-eappéd chickadee, the black-crowned night heron, and other birds, a black crown patch is noticeably marked off. The Ear Patches.—The ear patches in birds are small, yet often specially marked out by white boundaries, which are permanent parts of the pattern. Yet there is no doubt but that the acquisition of such white boundaries is a derived character. It is common for the ear patches to be colored differently from the surrounding parts, forming as in some species of tanagers a black auricular area contrasted with the blue of the head and neck. Of particular interest in the present connection, however, are those cases in which a pigmented ear patch is more or less clearly marked off by a white line above it or below, or both. The superciliary stripe, so common in birds, is of course a development of a primary break above the patch, separating it from the crown patch. Where the stripe is narrow it is hard to say which patch has begun to be restricted, though often no doubt both are more or less involved. Thus the Garganey teal has a very wide white eye stripe, and in other species of ducks the whole side of the head may be white, indicating much greater restriction of pigment formation in con- tiguous patches. A beautiful example of the develop- ment of a white stripe at the lower border of the ear patches is found in the Inca tern, in which a line of white feathers runs from just above the gape along the lower side of the auricular patch and separates it from the dark throat. But not only is the white line developed, but the feathers composing it are specially elongated and recurved, as if the mark were one of particular decora- tiveness. The dark ear patch is noticeable in many hawks, separated above and below by white areas, as in the duck hawk and the osprey, though differing in the size of the white areas. An instance in which the white line separating the ` crown patch from the ear patch, is even now in course of becoming established as part of the permanent pattern, No. 573] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 553 is afforded by the common guillemot (Uria troille) of the northern Atlantic. The other related species of the genus have the head and neck uniformly pigmented, but in U. troille a considerable proportion of specimens show a narrow white eyebrow and a postorbital line, in exactly the situation of the stripe in the albino robin previously noted, though not so broad nor so extended. Birds so marked were formerly considered a distinct species—the ringed murre (Uria ‘‘ringvia’’)—or perhaps a plumage of U. troille, and much effort has been made to determine their exact status. Both plumages are found in the same colonies and the two sorts of birds are known to have mated together (Müller, 1862). Verrill estimated that about 40 per cent. of the nesting birds he saw on the Labrador coast were of this variety, but this is probably a rather high estimate. I am convinced that the true explanation of this puzzling variation is that incipient albinism has gained a foothold, of such nature that areal restriction of the ear or crown patches is developing, so that a white line results between them. In the crested auklet (Æthia) a member of the same family, of the Pacific Coast, such a line has become fixed so that it now forms a characteristic mark of the species. In the case of the ‘‘ringed murre,’’ I should expect to see the eye stripe in the young as well as in the adult stage of those individuals which are to have the mark—in other words it is a permanent trait. No doubt the heredity of this white stripe is of some definite sort, and if a reces- sive character, it may nevertheless in time become com- mon to- an increasing number of birds, as this is a colonial species and the possibility of inbreeding is thus increased. The Neck Patches.—In birds the neck patches extend forward from the breast to meet the crown patch at the occiput and the ear patches at the sides of the head, thence ventrally to include the throat and chin. A study of albinistie pigeons, as previously noted, indicates that the neck patches are two separate areas of pigmentation, 554 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII one on each half of the part covered, with an ultimate center at the base of the neck, usually the last spot to re- main when the area is much reduced. In albinistic individuals, that is, those in which restric- tion of the pigment areas has taken place, the neck patches are usually first reduced at the upper part of the throat, so that a white patch appears from the chin to upper throat, as commonly seen in street pigeons; in others, however, the restriction may be at the posterior end of the patch, so that a white ring develops at the base of the neck. In many birds the neck patches have been much devel- oped as characteristic pigmented areas. Two general categories may be here distinguished: (1) those in which the neck is rather uniformly colored all about, and (2) those in which the ventral portion is heavily pigmented and the dorsal portion much less so. In the latter belong such birds as the black-capped ehickadee (Penthestes atricapillus) with a black throat but a pale neck. So, too, the golden-winged warbler (Vermivora chrysop- tera). In this latter category it is probable that a sec- ond factor is present, comparable to that producing a centrifugal type of pigmentation in mammals, such for example as in the Himalayan breed of rabbit, which has the end of the nose and the feet black-pigmented, contrary to the usual rule of normal areal reduction where the extremities are the first to become white. That this is a separate category from a physiological standpoint is indicated by its behavior in heredity as worked out so admirably by Faxon (1913) in the case of the Brewster’s warbler. He discovered that the black throat as present in the golden-winged warbler is recessive in the cross with a related species, the blue-winged warbler (Ver- mivora pinus), a yellow-throated bird. The offspring of this cross have white throats,—the so-called V. leuco- bronchialis. The black throat patch may be evidence of ‘‘centrifugal’’ pigmentation as defined farther on (p. 53). The essential bilaterality of such a throat patch is No. 573] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 555 further shown by the fact that one half only may be pres- ent as in the golden-winged warbler recorded by Dr. C. W. Townsend (1908). The first category, in which the neck is uniformly pig- mented is illustrated by many of the duck tribe, and probably involves the normal primary patches only. The primary patches are usually restricted first antero- ventrally producing a white throat. Often this is carried dorsally so as to form a white ring around the upper part of the neck by the separation of the neck patch from the crown and the ear patches. Again, if the neck patches are restricted posteriorly a white ring is formed at the base of the neck, a common permanent character in many species. The peculiar little goose-like bird— Nettapus, of India—has developed this type of marking so that its white neck is encircled by a narrow black ring, and the Labrador duck (Camptorhynchus) has a nearly similar mark (Fig. 57). Other ducks, e. g., the mallard, have the white ring at the base of the neck, only. In an interesting paper on the geese occurring in Cali- fornia, Swarth (1913) has pointed out that in the cack- ling goose (Branta c. minina) there is much variation in the amount of white on the head and neck. Figs. 58 to 62 are traced from a series of photographs illustrating this paper and show the throats of five specimens. The wide range of variation in these specimens indicates to my mind that this goose is in process of reducing the neck patches, and thereby developing a white collar, such as is present in the mallard, and perhaps also a white throat. The usual condition seen in Branta canadensis and in so-called normal specimens of B. c. minina is seen in Fig. 58. The white cheeks have been developed long ago in the history of the species, in part perhaps by the depigmentation of the ear patches. Now a second change is taking place in one of its subspecies. Thus in Figs. 59, 6l and 62, the neck patches have been reduced poste- riorly, a varying amount in each case. In Figs. 60, 61 and 62 these patches have been restricted anteriorly pro- 556 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII ducing a white throat, and as sometimes in the pigeon, imperfectly, so that a little island of pigment is cut off just at the chin. It is also obvious from these figures, that reduction may take place either at one end or the other, or at both ends in different individuals. The ulti- mate development of this line of reduction will produce i: a a | bo Gr b3 Fics. 58-62. VARIATIONS IN THE DEVELOPMENT OF THE NECK PATCHES IN THE CACKLING Goose (after Swarth). the narrow black collar seen in Nettapus previously men- tioned. It is worth noting also that in this goose the limits of the neck patch are by their black color sharply defined posteriorly from the gray of the breast which is pigmented from the shoulder patches. The Shoulder Patches.—The shoulder patches appear to center near the base of the wing, and in reduction produce white remiges, such as appear in a domesticated race of guinea fowl, as well as a white breast. The domesticated guinea fowl often shows this white area in ' the midline of the breast as the pigment areas fail to spread ventrally. In the normal pattern of wild birds, however, white wings are seldom seen except among cer- tain sea birds. White wing patches are often developed, but these are frequently only bars on pigmented feathers as in the goat-suckers. Probably among small land birds much white in the large wing feathers is a disadvantage, No. 573] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 557 and so not much developed. It is noticeable that white patches in the wing are often of such a nature that they are concealed through the folding of the wings when the bird is at rest. This accords with my belief that while in flight the bird is unavoidably conspicuous by reason of its motion, and that white patches showing at such times add little or nothing to the disadvantage. In the hairy and the downy woodpeckers (Dryobates), a white stripe down the back is developed as part of the pattern, and no doubt as in many mammals, marks the separation between the pigment areas of opposite sides. Centrifugal pigmentation is seen in some species as the kittiwake in which the outer primaries are black. The side patches are commonly continuous with those of the shoulders, and when ventrally restricted, give a white abdomen. Their median separation dorsally, is seen in the hairy and downy woodpeckers as above noted. I have not studied any special developments of these areas, and they are commonly small. The Rump Patches.—In birds as in. mammals the two rump patches pigment the posterior extremity of the body. Their ultimate centers are dorsal and so close to- gether that it is much less common for them to be sepa- rated medially than to be restricted laterally. With a slight areal reduction, a separation takes place between them and the side patches dorsally, so that a white area on the rump results. Often this white area represents doubtless a slight restriction of both sets of pigment patches which by drawing farther apart increase the white area along the lower part of the back. In the domestic pigeon much variation may be found, from a condition in which the lower back is wholly pigmented to one in which it is mostly white. The primary break which causes this white patch has been much developed in many groups of birds as a particular mark in the pattern. In many species it is simply of a paler hue than the surrounding parts as in the yellow-rumped warbler (Dendroica coronata) or the pine grosbeak (Pinicola). * 558 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vor. XLVII In others the tendency to albinism thus expressed has gone farther so that a pigmentless spot is formed. This white rump patch is present in many unrelated groups of birds in which it has independently arisen through parallel development. Thus it is seen in many of the smaller petrels, in the palm swift, the flicker woodpecker, the white-rumped and other sandpipers, the white- rumped shrike, the European house martin and others. The tail feathers are pigmented by these patches, and among various species show many steps in the process of pigment reduction. As in the domestic pigeon, occa- sional albinistic individuals show white outer tail feathers, in accordance with the rule that the first pig- ment reduction takes place at those parts of the primary areas that are farthest removed from the pigment centers. I have seen a white outer tail feather in wild specimens of song sparrows and Lincoln’s sparrow and it is occa- sional in other species. In others again this mark has become developed and fixed as a species character. Thus in the bay-winged bunting (Powcetes gramineus) there is a single white outer feather on each side, in the junco (Junco hyemalis) there are two. A white central tail feather is much rarer, but a pure white tail is found occa- sionally as in the hummingbird, Leucuria phalerata, the bald eagle and certain gulls, due to the permanent reduc- tion of the pigment area of the rump at this extremity. I once examined an albino ruffed grouse (Bonasa) which was entirely white except for a single feather among the upper tail coverts at the left side of the rump. This blemish in the otherwise pure white bird seemed inexpli- eable to those who examined it with me, but it merely represents the last remnant of the left-hand rump patch, still persisting though all the other pigment centers were inactive. It is very interesting that the white rump mark, so commonly found in unrelated ‘groups of birds, is one which is conspicuous in flight only, and the same is true of many of the white tail marks, such as outer white > No. 573] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 559 feathers that disappear when the tail is shut. This points to the conclusion that the development of a white mark which is ever conspicuous is allowed in nature in such cases only where it may be no detriment to the species through rendering it too conspicuous by contrast. Thus the bald eagle or the black-backed gull have nothing to fear from such a banner mark. For small weak-flying birds, however, the case may well be different. Yet even these often show much white and I believe that it would be possible for a species in its phylogeny to develop more and more white if at the same time its habits of watchful- ness or other actions developed equally to counteract any disadvantageous result that might accompany the in- crease. No doubt also a psychic factor is involved, com- parable to what among ourselves we call ‘‘fashion.’’ Thus a change in action or dress which departs too far from the accustomed appearance is apt to be disliked at first, though in time it may if persisted in, be tolerated and at length accepted. In the development of white markings, for example in the feathers of the tail, it seems likely that a series of small steps must have been made rather than too great and sudden changes. So in the rock pigeon the white of the tail is limited to the outer vane of the outer tail feather. In the turtle dove the outer vane of the outer feather, and the entire tips of the four outer feathers are white. The next step would be to develop an entirely white outer feather and then two (as in the passenger pigeon) and so on. In the sparrows similar steps are shown by the lark sparrow (Chon- destes) in which the tips only of the outer feathers are white, the bay-winged bunting which has practically all the outer feather white, and a little of the tip of the second, the junco with two outer feathers and part of a third white. No doubt steps such as these must have been passed through by many white-tailed species. It is difficult to say how disagreeable to their normally colored neighbors, albino birds may be. I have seen an albino robin in the fall of the year with a flock of other 560 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII robins and a white-spotted bee-eater with a flock of its brethren, in both cases wholly at peace. This of course was in flocking time when the social spirit is strong. The song sparrow (Melospiza) with white outer tail feathers, previously mentioned, was attacked and driven off by another song sparrow. In the Journal of the Maine Ornithological Society (Vol. 6, p. 48, 1904), C. H. Clark writes of a pair of albino eave swallows (Petrochelidon lunifrons), at Lubee, Maine, among a large colony of the common ones who seemed greatly annoyed at the albinos’ presence and fought with them until they finally killed one ... or rather injured it so badly that it died soon after. I also have a note of a white robin at Montclair, N. J., which in early July, 1909, was seen to be much beaten and driven about by another robin and eventually flew at full speed against a tree and was killed. CENTRIFUGAL COLORATION ' In addition to the primary pigment patches which I have discussed at some length, and the speckled condition r ‘‘Einglish’’ marking, there is, as I have already inti- mated, a third condition in which pigment is developed at the extremities or points. Itmay be called a centrifugal type and is almost the reverse of the centripetal or ‘‘pri- mary-patch’’ class. The two latter types of pigmentation may both be found in the same individual, but ordinarily this is not evident except in cases where the primary patches are somewhat restricted in area. It then may become appar- ent that pigment is present at exactly those points where, in the centripetal type of coloring, it is first to be lacking. Moreover it persists strongly, even though the primary areas are much reduced or largely absent. Curiously this sort of pigment seems almost always to be black. Apparently centrifugal pigmentation does not occur in all species. I have never seen any trace of it in dogs. In the house cat it is frequent, however. Thus in Figs. No. 573] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 561 18 and 19 it appears at the end of the tail. In the former figure the sacral patches are much reduced, though pres- ent, and together spread nearly half the length of the tail. The terminal half, or less, of the tail, however, is dark- pigmented, and a break occurs between the two sorts of markings, due to the failure of the centripetal patch to spread so as to unite with the centrifugal area. In Fig. 19 the sacral patches have wholly failed to develop but the centrifugal patch still covers the distal half of the tail. Possibly the dark heel marks in Fig. 16 are patches devel- oped in the same way. In the house cat, a dark or ‘*smutty’’ nose is often present in contrast to an other- wise. white face, or with the ear patches only slightly reduced. In the breed of rabbits known as ‘‘ Himalayan,” the centrifugal pigmentation remains, though the centri- petal markings have di ed, so that it is pure white except for the black nose, ear tips and toes. No doubt, however, it would be possible for the two types of pig- mentation to appear in a single individual. This is sug- gestive of the winter phase of the Arctic hares, in which the black ear tips contrast strongly with the otherwise white pelage. The physiology of the process whereby certain animals acquire a white winter coat is not yet fully worked ‘out. It is curious that in occasional melan- istic individuals of the eastern varying hare, the black color is retained throughout the winter, instead of being replaced by white—again a persistence of black pigment. In dappled gray horses a black patch sometimes appears on the bridge of the muzzle, usually the first place to show white in the restriction of centipetal pigmentation. The feet may also be black. Among certain antelopes a black muzzle mark is similarly present, and in Hunter’s ante- lope (Damaliscus hunteri) a white border partly sur- rounds such a mark, This, I believe, is due to a slight ‘restriction of the ear patches, sufficient to prevent them from reaching the muzzle, and of about the same nature as seen in the blesbok (Damaliscus albifrons) in which, through the absence of a centrifugal nose patch, the entire — a, a 562 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII front of the muzzle is white. The white chevron on the muzzle of several antelope (Strepsiceros, Taurotragus) is probably the result of a similar restriction of ear patches combined with a centrifugal nose patch, leaving a white line between. The black dorsal stripe seen in many mammals and the black tail tip are probably mani- festations of centrifugal pigmentation. The latter mark is common in stoats (Mustela) and among those that change to a white coat in winter, as the ermine, the tail tip still remains black. In sundry other genera, as Genetta, a black tail tip is part of the normal pattern. In their paper on albinistic negroes, Simpson and Castle (1913) published some highly interesting photo- graphs of ‘‘piebald’’ individuals. In four persons of one negro family the hair over the median part of the head from the occiput to forehead is pure white, as though due to a restriction of the aural pigment patches. In addi- tion, more or less of the median area of the back, as well as the hands (including much of the forearms) and feet (including the lower part of the ankle) are pigmented. These latter areas may represent centrifugal pigmenta- tion, but it should be noted that this is present in the dermis. Possibly there is a close relation between dermal pigment and that produced in the centrifugal style of pigmentation. Among birds, the black of the outer tail feathers of the ptarmigan (Lagopus) may be comparable. A black area is also sometimes present on the middle of the throat, or as in certain gulls the outer primaries may be black. This form of pigmentation is not found universally and the conditions governing its appearance are unknown, though its heredity in the ‘‘Himalayan’’ rabbit has been somewhat studied by Professor Castle. SuMMARY The principal points of this paper may be summed up as follows: . 1. In mammals and birds that normally are com- No..573] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT 563 pletely pigmented, there are certain definite points of the body from which as centers the tendency to develop pigment in the epidermal structures may become less and less. Outward from each of these centers pigment formation spreads to include very definite areas which in wholly pigmented animals overlap slightly at their borders or are at least contiguous. 2. A reduction in the area covered by any of these primary patches results in a white mark at the line of junction of two contiguous color patches, where no pig- ment is produced. These white marks between the pri- mary patches are spoken of as primary breaks. 3. Through a study of the breaks in pied individuals of domesticated species of mammals and birds, the boundaries of the primary patches have been determined. These are homologous in the two groups and subject to - a certain amount of variation in different types. They are: a median crown patch unpaired, and five paired patches on the opposite sides of the body, which are named from the general areas they co ver, the ear, neck, shoulder, side and rump patches. Their limits are more precisely defined under the different species treated. 4. These patches are physiologically independent of each other and may be differently colored in the same individual. 5. Pied patterns among many wild species have been brought about through the areal reduction of these pig- ment patches in a definite way so that the white markings resulting as breaks between the reduced patches have become fixed and form a permanent part of the normal pattern. 6. In several wild species this development of white markings is shown to be even now taking place, but the amount of pigment reduction is still fluctuating so that the white markings vary much in extent with different individuals. 7. The development of such white markings takes place probably by little and little, so that the departure from 564 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII type is not so great as to arouse antagonism against the varying individual on the part of others of its species. Also, the gradualness of the change allows the species to become accommodated to any ESN | that might concomitantly arise. , 8. The converse of this centripetal style of pigmenta- tion is present in many species, and results in pigmenta- tion (commonly black) at the extremities or along lines where primary breaks occur in the centripetal form, namely at the tip of the nose, ears, tip of the tail or the toes; possibly the black dorsal stripe is due also to centri- fugal pigmentation. Patterns may develop as in certain antelopes by a white break between patches of the two types. 2 In conclusion, I wish to express my indebtedness to Professor W. E. Castle for much helpful criticism and ` advice, and to the Museum of Comparative Zoology for permission to make record of specimens in its study collection. REFERENCES Allen, G. M. 1904. The Heredity of Coat Color in Mice. Proc. Amer. Acad. Arts and _ Sei., Vol. 40, pp. 61-163. Brewer, W. On ‘the Disposition of Color-markings of Domestic Animals. mer. Assoc. Adv. Sci., Vol. 30, pp. 246-251. Butler, A. x 1888. Notes Concerning Albinism among Birds. Jour. Cincinnati Soc. Nat. Hist., Vol. 10, pp. 1888a. pro in the Cuvier Club Collection. Jour. Cincinnati Soc. Nat. + Vol. 10, pp. 216-217. Castle, W. k p MacCurdy, H.; also Simpson, Q. I. Cory, C. B. 1912. The Mammals of Ilinois and Wisconsin. Field Mus. Nat. Hist., Zoo t 1L TARTERA : Some a of Albinism. Ornithologist and Oologist, Vol. 9, p. 48. ee, E. 1878. Melanism of Turdus migratorius. Bull. Nuttall Orn. Club, Vol. 3, pp. 47-48. No. 573] PATTERN DEVELOPMENT ; 565 Deane, R. 1876. Sr te and Melanism among North American Birds. Bull. Nuttall Orn. Club, Vol. 1, pp. 20-24. 1880. Additional Cases of Albinism and Melanism in North American Birds. Bull. Nuttall Orn. Club, Vol. 5, p. 25 (also 1879, pp. 26- ) 30, Vol. 4 Faxon, W. 1913. Brewster’s Warbler i ge leucobronchialis) a Hybrid between the Golden-winged Warbler (Helminthophila chrysop- a) and the Blue- pept Warbler "read dag pinus). . Mus. Comp. Zeol., Vol. 40, pp. 316, Hoffman, ap Hi 1878. Remarks upon Albinism in Several of Our Birds. AMER. NAT., Vol. 12, pp. 474—476 Keller, C. A. 1893. Evolution of the Colors of North American Land Birds. Occa- sional Papers Calif. Acad. Sci., No. 3, xii + 361 pp., 19 pls. Lawrence, G. N. 1889. Remarks upon Abnormal Coloring of Plumage Observed in Several Species of Birds. Auk, Vol. 6, pp. 46-50. ES C. C. 1914. ‘‘Dominant’’ and ‘‘Recessive’’ Spotting in Mice. AMER. NAT., Vol. 48, pp. 74-82. sare . A, 1885 Albiniem. Auk, Vol. 2, pp. 113-114. MacCurdy, H., and Castle, W. E. 1907. S élection and Groes-broading in Relation to the Inheritance of oat-pigments and Coat Patterns in Rats and Guinea-pigs. Carnegie Inst. Washington, Publ. 70, iii + 50 pp., plate. T 8. H. ©. 1862, Faerornes fuglefauna med bemaerkninger om _ fuglefangsten. Jidensk. Meddelels, Copenhagen, 1862, pp. 1-78. . Pearl, R. 1914. On Aah Results of Inbreeding a Mendelian Population: a Correc- and Extension of Previous Conclusions, AMER, NAT., Vol. i pp. 57 Pearl, R., and Boring, Alice M. 1914. ‘Bonk paoe KX Observations Regarding Plumage Patterns. Science, New Ser., Vol. 39, pp. 143-144. Pocock, R. I. 1907. On the Black- Fa Tan Pattern of Domestic Dogs (Canis fa- miliaris), Ann, Mag. Nat. Hist., Ser. 7, Vol. 19, pp. 192-194. 1909. On the Colors of Horses, Coes and Tapirs, Ann. Mag. Nat. Hist., Ser. 8, Vol. 4, pp. 404-415. Ramaley, F, - 1912. Mendelian Proportions and the Increase of Recessives. AMER. Nar., Vol. 46, pp. 344-351. a Simpson, Q. I., and Castle, W. E. , 1913. A Family o of a Negroes. AMER. NAT., Vol. 47, pp. 50-56, Figs. i z 566 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII Stone, W. 1912, The Phylogenetic Value of Color Characters in Birds. Jour. Acad. Nat. Sci. Phila., Ser. 2, Vol. 15, pp. 311-319, pl. 27. Strong, R. M. 1904. The Metallic Colors of Feathers from the Sides of the Neck of the Domestie Pigeon. Mark Anniv. Vol., New York, pp. 263-277, lL 2 pl. 20. 1905. Causes of Blue and Green in Feathers. Biol. Bull., Vol. 8, pp. 237-238 Swarth, H. S. 1913. A Study of a Collection of Geese of the Branta canadensis Group from the San Joaquin Valley, comic Univ. of Calif. Publ., Zool., Vol. 12, pp. 1-24, pl. 1-2, 8 text-figs. Sweet, D. A. 1907. Notes from Avon PETEN juncos from Maine]. Jour. Maine Ornith. Soc., Vol. 9, p. i G., and A. H. 909. Coneealing Coloration in the Animal Kingdom. New York. =a tee C: H 1883. Some Albinos in the Museum of the Philadelphia Academy. Bull. Nuttall Orn. Club, Vol. 8, p. 126. Townsend, C. W. 1908. On the Status of Brewster’s biegi PESIS leuco- bronchialis). Auk, Vol. 25, pp. 65-6: Ward, H. L. 1908. A Rapid Melanistic and Subsequent Partial Albinistic Change in a Caged Robin. Bull. Wisconsin Nat. Hist. Soc., Vol. 6, pp. 43—47. Worthen, C. K. 1897. Albinism, Melanism and Hybridism. Osprey, Vol. 1, pp. 23-24. SHORTER ARTICLES AND CORRESPONDENCE THE BEARING OF THE SELECTION EXPERIMENTS OF CASTLE AND PHILLIPS ON THE VARIABILITY OF GENES CAsTLE and Phillips have recently reviewed the results of six years’ work in which they selected for and against ‘‘hoodedness’’ in rats.! In ‘‘hooded”’ or ‘‘piebald’’ rats only part of the coat is pigmented ; the area of dark (versus white) coat varies greatly in different animals, but tends, in those of medium grade, to cover the head, shoulders and middle of the back, like a hood. Starting with a strain which was probably hybrid, although of unknown ancestry, and selecting during thirteen generations for a larger extent of colored coat (‘‘plus’’ selection), they suc- ceeded in obtaining animals with a greater and greater area of pigmentation. The average, the mode, and the extremes were raised. Conversely, selection for less pigmentation (‘‘minus”’ selection) was accompanied by a gradual but decided and continual dimunition in the dark area. ‘‘Return’’ selection also succeeded ; that is, plus selection was effective even in a line which was already lighter than the average on account of a previous minus selection, and, vice versa, minus selection caused a lightening of a strain that had been made exceptionally dark by a prior plus selection. Certain crosses proved that more than one factor affecting hoodedness is involved in the difference between the different races. Therefore the production of animals of desired grade by selection may perhaps be explained as a mere sorting out, into different lines of descent, of different combinations of the various factors for hoodedness originally present in the heterozygous ancestors. It is the opinion of Castle and Phillips, however, that this explanation will not suffice to account fully for the continued efficacy of selection in their experiments, and they believe it probable that a factor or factors for hoodedness are undergoing variation of a fluctuating nature. 1 Castle and Phillips, ‘‘Piebald Rats and Selection, An experimental test of the effectiveness of selection and of the theory of gametic purity in Mendelian crosses.’? Published by the Carnegie Institution of Washing- ton. See also Castle’s ‘‘ Pure Lines and Selection’’ in American Breeders’ Magazine, 1914. 567 568 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vot. XLVIII A conelusion so radical and so opposed to previous work should not be accepted, however, as long as it remains at all reasonably possible to use instead an explanation in harmony with the results of Johannsen and other investigators. Johann- sen dealt with a character—dimensions of seed—which must be- yond any doubt have been partially dependent upon a very great many factors, yet he found that selection had no effect whatever after he had separated the different genotypes from one another. Thus he proved the constancy of a great many genes ‘‘at one blow’’—namely, of all the genes appreciably concerned in seed size. Of course, if there had been a chance for cross-fertilization in his experiments, he, like Castle, would have obtained a result from selection, but this would have been due to recombination, not variation, of genes. All our evidence points to the conclusion that the vast majority of genes are extremely constant, although they differ somewhat in that very slight amount of variation which they do show. For example, in Drosophila, although in the case of most genes not more than one mutation has been found, yet in one case (possibly in two or three cases) a locus has mutated three times, each time in a different way, thus giving rise to a system of multiple allelomorphs containing four mem- bers. This gene evidently is more subject to mutation than the others, yet this formation of a series of multiple allelomorphs can not even remotely be compared to fluctuating variability, for the three mutations were all large steps (much smaller could easily have been detected), and they were found only during the exami- nation of some millions of individuals in the rest of which the locus was not observed to mutate at all. Some few genes are known, however, which really do change frequently (e. g., that for ‘ Gapiaghted?? corn), but these cases are extremely rare; moreover, here the degree and nature of the change are fixed, and also, after the change has once occurred the instability of the gene is lost. Thus, in no known ease do the variations of a gene among, let us say, several thousand immediate descendants of the individual possessing it, form a probability curve, as neo-Dar- winians might perhaps suppose, nor even are any cases known where genes can undergo frequent changes that may vary at all in kind or amount or occur successively. Let us then inquire into the probability and gdeanay” of that explanation of Castle and Phillips’s results which does not require the assumption that a gene or genes involved change compara- No.573] SHORTER ARTICLES AND CORRESPONDENCE 569 tively frequently and suecessively, but which assumes a sort- ing out of numerous factors. It is now pretty generally ac- cepted by Mendelians that the germ plasm of any of the higher organisms contains a large number of genes, which play vari- ous rôles in the numberless processes and reactions of devel- opment whereby the egg is transformed into the adult indi- vidual. The exact nature and intensity of any one characteristic of this adult organism (e. g., hoodedness in rats) is dependent upon the nature of each of the various reactions which were involved in producing this character, and thus dependent upon all the genes (and environmental factors also) involved in any of those reactions. Now, in an ordinary Mendelian cross, all the individuals are usually homozygous and alike in respect to all but one of the pairs of genes that noticeably affect the character concerned. In such a case, then (so far as differences in environ- mental influences do not obseure the outcome), one obtains the simple Mendelian results derived from the segregation, at reduc- tion, and recombination, at fertilization, of but this one pair of allelomorphs. The strain of hooded rats, however, was probably a hybrid between two races of rather remote relationship. When two such races are crossed, the individuals often differ in more than one pair of those factors that affect the character studied, especially if the character is such as to be influenced. by a relatively large number of genes, It can not be questioned that some characters are thus determined or influenced by a much larger number of developmental reactions than are others, and such characters will therefore vary more in inheritance, since if a difference exists between two individuals in respect to any given gene, these characters are more likely to be affected than others. Gross size, for example, is a character dependent in this way upon an exceptionally large number of genes, for any gene which influ- ences the size of any organ must affect to some extent the total size. In some other cases in which characters are found to be influenced by relatively many genes, the reason for this is not- so evident, e. g., in the case of the red flower-color of flax, or the truncated condition of the wing in some races of Drosophila.. Here the production of the character may be conceived to be | dependent upon some reaction that can be easily modified by various means.? For our present purpose we must assume that 2It is conceivable that differences in respect to numerous genes hav ve: sometimes arisen even in the ¢ ease of characters not naturally very B ; 570 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII the character ‘‘hoodedness’’ belongs in this class and that the ancestral hooded rats used by Castle and Phillips were the de- scendants of a cross involving many genes for that character. The results of such a cross are of course complicated, for the different pairs of allelomorphs generally can undergo recombina- tion at the reduction division of the hybrid, so that in F, or subsequent generations as many different genetic types of indi- viduals are formed as there are possible different combinations of those factors wherein the ancestors differed. Not all these genetic types, of course, will fall into different phenotypes, yet generally there will be a large number of overlapping pheno- types among the progeny. The larger the number of factors in which the two ancestral lines differed, the larger will be the number of different possible combinations of these factors, and accordingly the smaller will be the chance of any individual having one of those particular combinations necessary to a relatively high or a relatively low intensity of the character. In other words, the larger the num- ber of factors (for one character) for which a population is heterogeneous, the more numerous are the possible different grades of intensity of this character among the different indi- viduals, but the fewer will be the individuals which approach the more extreme grades theoretically possible in such a population.® Suppose, for example, that two parents differ in five pairs of factors for hoodedness, which are partially dominant* to their allelomorphs and summative in their action. Then in F, not one influenced by diverse means, merely because one of ae two races had been subjected to a very long and drastic selection, so that any of those rare mutations which affected that character in the desired es had in thi race been preserved. Selection in such a case, however, would have to in- volve many millions of orn ual 3 One extreme, e. g., the ‘plan,’ ’ will be rather frequent, however, if all the ‘*plus’’ factors dominate completely. But in the case of the hooded rats we must assume either that dominance is generally incomplete or that in the case of some factors the ‘‘minus’’ allelomorph dominates in the case of others the ‘‘plus,’’ since F, rats from a cross of the plus by the minus strain are on the average intermediate in type between these two extremes #It is of course by no means necessary to assume incomplete dominance of the factors. If dominance is complete (in some cases the ‘‘minus’’ fac- r may dominate, in others the ‘‘plus’’), the rigor of selection will be di- minished, since heterozygous forms can not be distinguished from homozy- us. Therefore, although a somewhat greater number of individuals will be found having the limiting values, it will take longer to bring the average up to the limit. No.573] SHORTER ARTICLES AND CORRESPONDENCE 571 individual in a thousand will have the most extreme dark or light grade of hoodedness possible. However, by selecting the more extreme individuals, and mating them together, a still more extreme grade of hoodedness may be obtained in F, (both as to average and limiting values), and the same process may be con- tinued for a good many generations. The number of generations during which effective selection is possible depends on the num- ber of factors concerned, the rigor of selection, and the amount of inbreeding of brother to sister. In regard to the latter point, since brother and sister are much more apt to be alike in their genetic constitution than are other individuals, offspring from such a mating are more apt to be homozygous and alike, or, we may say, such offspring will tend to be homozygous and alike in a larger number of factors; then, mating two individuals homozygous for these factors together, there will be much less variation and so less opportunity to con- tinue selection among their progeny. In the case of Castle and Phillips’s experiments, however, no such attempt at inbreeding was reported. Here, then, the individuals mated together would be more apt to differ genetically, even though they looked alike (thus, one might be AA bb, the other aA bB), and their descendants would therefore present a larger number of different combinations of factors for the selector. Often a greater effect may be eventually. produced in this manner than by inbreeding, for a larger number of combinations of factors are thus pro- duced, some of which may be of more extreme type. The effect would usually be slower, however, since such matings tend to keep the strain heterozygous and are often steps backwards. Cross-breeding, then, will help to explain the relatively slow but long-continued and eventually large effect of selection in Castle and Phillips’s experiments, although such a result could also be obtained without cross-breeding if the factors were numerous enough, The ‘‘return selections’? also are easily explicable on the multiple factor view. Due to the original difference in so many factors, and the fact that cross-breeding diminishes the tendency to homozygosis which selection favors, the rats were presumably heterozygous even after generations of selection. They would not be as heterozygous as before, of course, and, correspondingly, Castle and Phillips did find less variation in the rats after selec- tion. Yet there would still be a good chance for recombination, 572 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST — [Vou. XLVIII and an alteration in the race could therefore be produced by further selection or by return selection. As we have seen, this is especially true if certain factors are completely dominant, al- though dominance is by no means a necessary condition. As a very simple illustration, let us suppose that the ‘‘plus’’ factors A and B dominate over the ‘‘minus’’ factors ‘‘a’’ and ‘*b,’’ respectively, and each increase the pigmented area to about the same extent. To begin with, two moderately hooded indi- viduals, Aa bb and aa Bb, were mated together. They produced laa bb—light-hooded, laa Bb and 1Aa bb—both moderate, and lAa Bb—dark. We first select for dark; mating the dark rats together, 9 darks, 6 moderates, and 1 light, would be pro- duced (F,). The average color of the offspring has thus been increased by selection (the limiting color, too, if dominance is incomplete). It can be still further increased in subsequent generations. On the other hand, the color can be made lighter again by a ‘‘return selection,’’ for if, instead of mating the F, or F, darks together, we mate the moderates or mate darks with moderates, many of the matings will give offspring lighter, on the average, than in the preceding generation; e. g., Aa Bb by Aa bb - gives 3 dark, 4 moderate, 1 light, as compared with the previous 9 dark, 6 moderate, 1 light. In subsequent generations, the average could be brought still lower. Let us now see whether there is any experimental evidence in support of the multiple factor explanation of Castle and Phillips’s results, aside from the fact that it is adequate and is the only one consistent with other work. One point of evidence we have noted—the variability of the rats continued to decrease as a result of selection in either direction. This we should of course expect on the multiple factor view, for selection gradually tends towards homogeneity in a population, even though it may require a long time to produce complete homogeneity. The second and strongest evidence is from crosses. The crosses show that one of the factors concerned in differ- entiating hooded rats from wild rats, which are pigmented all over, or from ‘‘Irish’’ rats, which are almost completely pig- mented, is ‘‘hypostatie.’’ In other words, a rat having the normal allelomorphs of this factor will always be self-colored, or nearly so; one having the other allelomorphs will always be distinctly hooded, although the amount of the hoodedness varies. “Self, ”’ as it happens, is dominant, in this case, over hooded. ae No.573] SHORTER ARTICLES AND CORRESPONDENCE 513 Thus, on crossing a hooded to a wild or Irish rat, all the F, are self (or nearly so) ; in F, there are three selfs to one hooded, but the hoodeds vary in intensity. The question then is, does this variation (so far as it is not due to ‘‘environmental’’ differences) depend upon what other ‘‘epistatic’’ or ‘‘modifying’’ factors for hoodedness may or may not be present, or is there evidence that it depends instead, or in addition, upon a variability of one or more of the factors for hoodedness? As will be shown below, it ean be proved that different combinations of modifying factors do occur in the different hooded indiviuals: this being true, there can be no ground for making the unusual postulate that in this case or in the selection experiments a factor or factors concerned undergo variation. | The proof is that when light hooded rats from the minus strain are crossed to wild or Irish rats the hooded rats in F, vary ` much more than did the original strain of hooded rats and aver- age much darker. Obviously, the P, hooded rats differed from the wild or Irish in a number of modifiers as well as in the hypo- static factor; moreover, as we should have expected, this differ- ence consisted chiefly in the fact that the wild or Irish rats con- tained ‘‘plus’’ allelomorphs in place of some of the ‘‘minus’”’ modifiers present in the P, strain that had undergone minus selection. Thus the F, hooded rats, containing various combina- tions of these modifying factors wherein the two strains differed, varied much more than did the parental strain of hooded rats, and were on the average much darker. In order to escape this conclusion that modifying factors were involved, Castle and Phillips at first postulated that the reason that the F, hooded were darker than the original ‘‘minus’’ strain was because the factor for hooded had in many cases become con- taminated by its allelomorph (the factor for self) in the F, rats. This is violating one of the most fundamental principles of genetics—the non-mixing of factors—in order to support a vio- lation of another fundamental prineiple—the constancy of fac- tors. The refutation of their supposition came unexpectedly soon. It would be expected, on the view of multiple factors, that the wild or Irish rats (containing the allelomorph for self in place of the hypostatie factor for hooded) would not possess as many ‘‘minus’’ modifiers as the hooded strain which had been Specially selected to contain as many of these as possible; neither would these ‘‘self’’ rats contain as many ‘‘plus’’ modifiers as the 574 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVII hooded strain which had undergone plus selection (and which so contained nearly all of the plus modifiers originally present in either the self or the hooded ancestors). Thus it was to be ex- pected that, just as a cross of self with the minus race gave F, hooded rats darker than the original minus strain, so a cross of wild or Irish rats with hoodeds resulting from the plus selection would give F, hooded rats lighter than those of the plus strain. This result was actually obtained. It was fatal to the idea that the difference between the P, strain of hooded rats and the F, hoodeds was due to contamination of the allelomorph for hooded with that for self, since such contamination should have resulted in F, hooded rats darker than those of P,, not lighter. For wild and Irish rats are both much more extensively pigmented than hoodeds even of the plus strain. The change in hoodedness from P, to F, was therefore due to recombinations of the modifying factors wherein the two ' strains differed. That many such modifiers were concerned is indicated by the evenly distributed variability of the F, hoodeds and the fact that very few were as extreme as the hooded grand- parents. The same fact is brought out in a cross of the minus with the plus race; here no clear-cut ratios were obtainable, the classification into different genotypes being rendered impossible by the multiplicity of factors (no one of which was hypostatic as in the other crosses). Of course, this knowledge of so many factors being concerned in the crosses helps our interpretation of the selection results decidedly, for the more numerous are the factors concerned, the longer would it be possible to continue an effective selection on the progeny of the hybrids, and the orig- inal hooded rats of the selection experiments were admittedly in all likelihood descended from just such hybrids. The exact num- ber and effect of the different factors can not be determined from Castle and Phillip’s data, since to do this very special crosses must be made and individual pedigrees kept. Selection experi- ments can be of little value so long as there are factors for which the individuals may be heterozygous, unless these factors can be accurately followed in inheritance. Of course, it is quite possible that in the course of these long- continued experiments mutations affecting the hoodedness occa- sionally happened to arise, especially since it seems likely that this character is dependent upon an unusually large number of genes, for then, as a matter of mere chance, any mutation which No.573] SHORTER ARTICLES AND CORRESPONDENCE 95175 occurred would be more likely to affect it than it would be to affect most characters. It is interesting to note that one such mutation, of a very marked and unquestionable character, was in fact observed. The mutant factor proved to be a strong ““plus’’ modifier, which was almost completely dominant, and itself showed no contamination or variation, so far as could be determined. It arose, as it happened, in the plus strain. A part of the effectiveness of selection may therefore have been due to the occurrence and sorting out of such occasional muta- tions, but there is no way of telling how many of these took place, or any need for assuming them at all in explaining the result. These rare mutations, however, would form a very different phe- nomenon from such fluctuating or frequent and progressive vari- ation of a gene or genes concerned as Castle postulates. Although the academic possibility of variation of the latter type can not be denied, there is no experimental evidence which can be used to support it, and there is good evidence against it in many individual eases. It is difficult to believe that this suggestion of Castle and Phillips was not made in a spirit of mysticism, when we con- sider also their suggestion that the genes may undergo contami- nation, and especially when we consider the following passage, with which their paper concludes: It seems to us quite improbable that the plus mutation could have arisen in the minus selection series. We believe that the repeated se- lection which was practised had something to do with inducing this change in the plus direction. If one ean increase at will the “ modi- fiers” which make the pigmentation more extensive, it does not seem strange that after a time a readjustment should oceur within the cell which should incorporate modifiers in that part of the cell which is re- sponsible for the unit-character behavior of the hooded pattern. This would amount to a quantitative change in the unit-charaeter for hooded pigmentation. To thus suppose that independent genes fuse or induce changes in one another, merely because they happen to produce similar end effects upon the organism, and in spite of the fact that they usually lie in different chromosomes and are apt to differ from each other as much as do other genes, is utterly teleological. A paper by A. L. and A. C. Hagedoorn criticizing Castle’s work and conclusions, appeared at the same time as the paper of 576 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (VoL. XLVIII Castle and Phillips.» The Hagedoorns champion the multiple factor hypothesis as an explanation of Castle’s results, and also cite certain rather inconclusive experiments of their own to sup- port this point of view. They err, however, in supposing that the factors concerned must be incompletely dominant; as we have seen, this is not a necessary assumption, if we admit that in the case of some modifiers the ‘‘minus’’ allelomorph dominates, in others the ‘‘plus.’’ They also err in denying the poaki, on the multiple factor view, of successful ‘‘return selection,” if inbreeding be strictly followed. In fact they offer this as a test of their point of. view. As we have seen, ‘‘return selection’? would be possible in some cases, even if the animals were inbred; and in Castle and Phillips’s experiments, where inbreeding was not followed, ‘‘return selection’’ was certainly very effective. Finally, papers have recently appeared by MacDowell,® in which he gives evidence that certain other cases of inheritance (e. g., head size in rabbits), formerly considered by Castle to support the idea of genic variation and contamination, are probably best interpreted on the view of multiple factors instead. His evidence consists in the fact that the characters concerned are somewhat more variable in the offspring of. back-crosses than in F,, as we should expect on the basis of recombination of multiple factors, but which he believes could not plausibly be explained otherwise. HERMANN J. MULLER SA. L. & A. C. gee Le ‘í Studies on Variation and Selection,’’ Zeit. f. ind. Abst. u. Verab., 6 E. C. MacDowell, aiei Factors in Mendelian Inheritance,’’ Jour. Exp. Zool., 1914, and Carnegie Inst. of Wash., 1914. VOL. XLVIII, NO, 574 OCTOBER, 1914 pk pend a5 _ THE AMERICAN NATURALIST A MONTHLY JOURNAL Devoted to the Advancement of the Biological Sciences with Special Reference to the Factors of Evolution CONTENTS Page Sex-limited and Sex-linked Inheritance. Professor T. H. MORGAN =- — 677 . Inheritance of Endosperm at in Sweet x hse oo of Maize. G. N. COLLINS and J. H ~ 684 2 G Res A Study of Variation in the Apple. W. J. YOUNG z Shorter Articles and Discussion: Variation and Correlation in the Mean Age at Marriage of Men and Women. RTHUR a Roxana H. VIVIAN. Duplicate Genes. SEWALL WRIGHT = - 635 Notes and Literature: A Study of Desert Vegetation, Professor CHARLES E. Bessey - = > a i a e | CE THE SOIENCE PRESS LANCASTER, PA. GARRISON, N. ¥. NEW YORK: SUB-STATION 84 The American Naturalist intended for piui and books, etc., intended for review should be MSS sent to Ba Editor of THE AM RICAN NATURAL rt articles saatat summaries of rese IST, Garrison-on-Hudson, New York. earch work bearing on the igor -i Sear evolution are especially welcome, and will be given preference in pos rea reprints of eaae are supplied to authors free of charge. con Further ‘eprint will be supplied a ons and E rer Sabi is four ees a aerea postage twenty- ents. adver visdaens should be sent to the publishers. ear. e ce as additional. The aiivertinine: rates are Four Dollars for a page. The Foreign postage is fifty cents pe The charge for single copies THE SCIENCE PRESS Lancaster, Pa. Garrison, N. Y. NEW YORK: Sub-Station 84 Entered as second-class matter, ipa 2, 1908, at the Post pery at Lancaster, Pa., under the Act of ngress of Mareh 3, 1 F E ARCTIC, ICELAND and GREENLAND BIRDS’ SKINS, Well Prepared Low Prices Particulars of G. DINESEN, Bird Collecto Husavik, North Iceland, Via aiie koai WANTED TO PURCHASE a set of BIRDS OF AMERICA by J. J. Audubon, 7 or 8 volumes, please report, stating cash price, stat- ing condition, binding and dates of volumes. F. C. HARRIS, Box 2244 Boston, Massachusetts For Sale Entire An important collection of Indian Birds’ Eggs, containing approximately 3,300 specimens of about 620 species, and in- | Marine Biological Laboratory cluding among other varieties the Green- | ish Willow Warbler (Acanthopneuste viridanus), Red-browed Finch (Calla- | t canthis burtoni), Tibetan Sand-Grouse | (Syrrhaptes tibetanus), Spotted Sand- Grouse (Pteroclurus senegallus), Ibis- | bill (Ibidorhynchus struthersi), White- bellied Minivet (Pericrocotus erythro- pygius), Bearded Vulture (Gypaétus barbatus), numerous Cuckoos with their hosts, etec., etc. Particulars and full list may be had from W. F. H. ROSENBERG 57 Haverstock Hill London, N. W. Woods Hole, Mass. INVESTIGA Facilities for research in Zoology, TION Embryology, Physiology and Bot- Entire Year any. Seventy-six private labora- Leste ges oe a for not over three tables se sre ae AA ia te eat Ses — wek ce werden the pe of m rs of the maf. The fee for su $50. urses of laboratory instru truction INSTRUCTION t laboratory ta tnverte- 1 to August 11, $50.00. A lecture course pe ag wg A ing E Animals ts, a pfeil Hiatt Shere Open the Entire Year wo THE AMERICAN NATURALIST Vout. XLVIII : October, 1914 No. 574 . SEX--LIMITED AND SEX-LINKED INHERITANCE PROFESSOR T. H. MORGAN CoLUMBIA UNIVERSITY Darwin used the expression ‘‘inheritance as limited by sex’’ to include all cases in which a character is peculiar to one sex. His list of such cases covers in the main the group of secondary sexual characters. Darwin’s expres- sion has been contracted to sex-limited inheritance, and is widely employed to-day in the same general sense in which Darwin used the expression. For instance, Bateson in his book ‘‘Mendel’s Principles of Heredity’’ includes both horns in sheep and color blindness in man as sex- limited characters. Now that the inheritance of several of these cases has been definitely worked out, it has become increasingly evi- dent that such characters as color blindness, and hemophi- lia in man, the twenty-five ‘‘sex-linked’’ characters in Dro- sophila, and certain characters in birds and in butterflies follow a law of inheritance that is essentially different from that followed by some of the other cases. It has become necessary, therefore, to recognize two groups of cases that differ fundamentally in regard to their heredity. To one of these groups I have applied the term sex-linked inheritance, and, for the present at least, we may still make use of the older expression sex-limited inheritance (and 1 See pp. 169-174 in section headed ‘‘ Heredity Limited by Sex; the Horns of Sheep,’’ where the term sex inheritance limited descent (p. 172) also appears. 577 578 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII sex-limited character) to cover that class of cases (obvi- ously a very mixed one which will be broken up as our knowledge regarding it becomes more certain) that in- cludes largely, as originally intended, the secondary sexual characters.? In those cases of sex-linked inheritance, in which the male is heterozygous for the sex factor, the grandfather transmits his peculiarity, through his daugh- ters, to half of his grandsons only; and reciprocally an affected female transmits her peculiarity to all her sons, and, through her sons bred to her daughters, to half of her granddaughters and to half of her grandsons’. More- over the appearance of the character in the female is not exceptional or abnormal, as is sometimes implied in cases like color blindness in man, for, the character can always be transferred from the male to the female by suitable crosses, On the other hand, there are cases in which a character appears in one sex only—the character is limited, there- fore, to the male or to the female. Such cases may be properly called sex-limited, and were so called by Darwin. As typical examples I may cite the horns of certain races of sheep that are present in the ram and absent in the 2G. H. Shull has recently said (Zeit. Ind. Abst. und Vererb., XII, 1914, p. 160) that, in his opinion, it would be better to retain the term sex-limited for those cases that I call sex-linked and call other cases secondary sexual characters. This view is not historically in accord with Darwin’s usage 0 e term ‘‘limited by sex.’’ This fact, in itself would be a sufficient argu- ment for rejecting Shull’s suggestion, but, in addition, the term sex limited is an actual misnomer for the class of cases to which he proposes to apply it. There are cases like the eosin eye of Drosophila that differ in the male and female in the same way as do many secondary sexual characters (in fact they are such in a descriptive sense) but nevertheless show sex-linked inhert- tance. Since a new name is required to express our fuller information regard to some of the characters that were mapa included under the older term, why not begin by adopting suita n 3 In those cases in which the female is paranda us for a sex factor, as in birds and in butterflies, the same principle is involved but the sequence is, in a sense, reversed; thus the grandmother transmits, through her sons, her peculiarity to half of her granddaughters; and reciprocally, the affected male transmits his peculiarity to all of his daughters, and, through his daughters bred to his sons, to half of his grandsons and to half of his grand- daughters, No. 574] INHERITANCE 579 ewe (or else more developed in the ram than in the ewe); the color of butterflies like Papilio Memnon, with three types of females; and the dark spot on the abdomen of the male of the bug Euchistus variolarius. These characters can not be transferred through the gametes to the female of their own race by any known combination. Whether one likes or does not like the particular terms used to denote these two classes of cases, the fact remains that there are two such categories, and to ignore their existence is only to make obscure a distinction that is per- fectly plain. Concerning the mechanism involved there is something more that may be said. It has been sufficiently shown in the case of sex-linked inheritance that the sex-linked char- acter follows the known distribution of the sex chromo- somes. It is unnecessary to repeat here the abundant evidence jn support of this statement. The simplest inter- pretation of this known relation is that the character is dependent for its realization on the sex chromosomes. I do not mean, of course, that the sex chromosomes alone- produce the character but that something in these chromo-. somes, some ‘‘factor,’’ acting in conjunction with the rest of the cell, conditions the character. On the other hand, in the ease of sex-limited characters the facts can not be explained on the assumption that the characters follow the sex chromosomes. It is clear that they do not do so. But we can give a consistent interpre- tation of the facts if we assume that sex-limited characters follow the distribution of the ordinary chromosomes. Since this relation has recently been not understood and misinterpreted I may be pardoned, I hope, for taking up the question once more. Wood crossed horned Dorset sheep with hornless Suf- folks. The sons had horns, the daughters lacked them. Inbred these gave in the F, generation—horned J, 3; hornless 3, 1; horned 9, 1; hornless ?, 3. Bateson and Punnett have shown that the results are explicable on the basis that one factor for horns in the male produces 580 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVII horns but one factor is insufficient in the females. This conclusion was put to the test by breeding an F, hornless ewe to a hornless ram. The F, ewe should be hetero- zygous for the factor for horns, and, therefore, when she is bred to a homozygous hornless ram, half of her off- spring should be heterozygous for hornlessness and half homozygous for hornlessness. Since half of her sons should have a factor for horns they are expected to develop horns, and this is what occurred. Half of the daughters also should have a factor for horns, but should not develop horns, and this also was true. It has been recognized for several years that this and related cases can not be explained on the assumption that the factors involved are carried by the X or by the Y chromosomes. But we can interpret the statement that one factor for horns is sufficient in the males to call forth horns, but not sufficient in the female ‘‘in terms of chromo- somes,’’ if a factor for horns is carried by one of the chromosomes other than the sex chromosome. In other words we need only appeal to a mechanism with which we are familiar to cover the results. The second illustration is furnished by the recent experiments of Foot and Strobell, and since the authors have rejected the chromosome hypothesis as inapplicable to their results, and since in the case of insects the condi- tions are simplified because castration experiments have shown that the sex glands are not themselves responsible for the secondary sexual characters, we may profitably consider this case even more fully. In one of the bugs, Euchistus variolarius, the male has a black spot on the abdomen. The female lacks the spot. A female of this species was crossed to a male of another species, viz., Huchistus servus, having no spot in either sex. The daughters had no spot, the sons had a spot fainter than that of variolarius. Inbred these gave, in F,, 249 females without a spot, 107 males with a spot (devel- oped to different degrees) and 84 males without a spot. The F, results show that one factor for spot in the male No. 574] INHERITANCE 581 suffices to call forth in some degree the spot in the hybrid. Its intensity varies from a condition approaching that in pure variolarwus to a faint spot (possibly even to no spot at all). The F, results show also that a single factor in the female fails to cause the spot to develop in that sex. In the F, male the failure of the spot to reach in most cases its full development shows obviously that the same conditions that produce a male that is perfect so far as his sex gonad is concerned, do not suffice to cause the full development of the spot, although the factor for the spot is present in one dose at least. The only confusion that is liable to arise is that in none of the F, females did the spot appear, although in some of them there must have been a double dose of spot. But the difficulty is imaginary as a little thought will show. In the first place the female of E. variolarius herself does not show the spot, yet this female must have a double dose of spot if spot is in the X chromosome or in any other chromosome (except the Y). Foot and Strobell by an elaborate analysis of the case show that the factor can not be carried by either the X or the Y chromosome. It is unnecessary to repeat their argument; for, if the factor were carried by the X chromo- some, only half of the grandsons should show it, while, in ' fact, many more than half of them show it; and it could not be carried by the Y chromosome because the Y chromo- some of variolarius is not present in the female, hence could not have entered the cross as made. We are con- cerned then only with a third possibility, viz., that there is something in the female condition itself that is inimical to the development of the spot. Since neither X nor Y carries the factor in question it must be present in duplex in the female of variolarius (if every gamete must have tt in simplex and the experiment shows that this is the case), and since the spot does not show in the female of vario- larius, it is obvious that it can not appear in that sex even | in duplex. If it be granted that the character is like other Mendelian characters, and the authors’ evidence show that it is inherited as are Mendelian characters, the conclusion 582 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII is self evident; for, in demonstrating that all of the gametes of variolarius carry spot the authors actually destroy their own argument. It only remains to point out some of the different ways in which a factor being present in duplex both in the male and in the female produces its effect only in the male. In some cases it has been shown that the ovary produces some substance that is inimical to the production of cer- tain characters. For instance in fowls and in ducks the presence of the ovary suppresses the development of the male plumage. That the factors for the male plumage are present is shown by its development when the ovary is removed. But in some insects it has been found that neither the ovary nor the testis produces these kinds of substances; for, when the testis or the ovary is removed the secondary sexual characters are not affected. Here the mode of explanation must be different. But the con- ditions, or complex, or factors that produce the ovary in the female are acting in every cell of the body, and con- sequently an effect, that is indirectly caused in the fowl or duck, might be directly caused in the insect. For, each cell is a chemical factory. Such a factory may help to produce an ovary and the ovary produce a substance that demonstrably suppresses the male plumage, or the same ' kind of factory may do similar work through the activity - of some other part of the body, or conceivably it may do its work in every cell of the body. This it seems to me is the most reasonable view to take of the matter in the case of the variolarius-servus cross. We can express the same thought in symbols by representing:the female of vario- larius by XXAABBCCDDSS, etc., and the male by XYAABBCCDDSS, ete. The chemical interaction be- tween two X’s and the rest of the cell is of such kind that it produces a female, and the female complex, as such, is inimical to the development of a spot and favorable for the development of the accessory organs of reproduction and of all secondary sexual characters of the female, while XY and the rest of the cell is inimical to the development No. 574] INHERITANCE 583 of the acessory organs and of the secondary sexual char- acters of the female, and favorable for the development of the accessory sexual organs and of the secondary sexual organs of the male. This view is of course compatible with the idea that there may be special factors for these organs in chromosomes other than the sex chromosomes, and the view holds both in a general way and on the special chromosome hypothesis as well. To assume that all the factors for characters that are shown by the male or by the female must be carried by a sex chromosome of some kind, if carried at all by chromo- somes, is a travesty of the point of view of those who hold to the chromosome hypothesis as a reasonable working hypothesis to account for Mendelian inheritance. Just as it has been shown that there are factors in the sex chromosomes that affect many parts of the body, that are not concerned with differences of sex; so, on the other hand, the evidence shows that there are factors in other chromosomes that are influential in producing secondary sexual characters. INHERITANCE OF ENDOSPERM TEXTURE IN SWEET x WAXY HYBRIDS OF MAIZE G. N. COLLINS anp J. H. KEMPTON U. S. DEPARTMENT OF AGRICULTURE INTRODUCTION In a previous publication,’ the first and second genera- tion of crosses between sweet and waxy varieties of maize were reported and a tentative explanation of their be- havior was suggested. It is now possible to add the results of the third season, which to some extent afford a test of the explanation proposed in our first publication. The immediate result of crosses between the Chinese variety of maize having a waxy endosperm and varieties with sweet endosperm was the production of seeds having a horny endosperm indistinguishable from that of ordi- nary field varieties of maize. In the second xenia genera- tion all three kinds of endosperm reappeared in the pro- portion of 9.20 horny, 3.95 sweet, and 2.85 waxy. This ratio was accepted as a 9:4:3 dihybrid ratio. For al- though the deviations of the individual ears, individual families and the totals were too large to be ascribed to chance, the deviations were not consistently in one direc- tion and to predicate more complicated formule would have necessitated different assumptions for different ears. The only interest in treating the problem in this way would be that of solving a mathematical puzzle, for it would be practically impossible to secure individuals enough to test adequately the validity of the assumptions which it would have been necessary to make. Admitting, then, that the ratios were only an approxi- mation representing a general tendency, it became of 1 Collins, G. N. and Kempton, J. H., ‘‘Inheritance of Waxy Endosperm in Hybrids with Sweet Corn,’’ Circular 120, U. S. Department of Agriculture, Bureau of Plant Industry, 1913. 584 No. 574] INHERITANCE 585 interest to learn whether predictions were still possible. For the purpose of making comparisons easy, the original diagram representing the second xenia genera- tion is here repeated. (See Fig. 1.) The meaning of the symbols is as follows: S is the factor for sweet, and X the factor for waxy. When both S and X are present the seed is expected to be horny. Small letters indicate the absence or latency of the factors. K SX a ETT KKA DEE SX” TKE Sr Ko =. e gig L Steel he aed ve a sS ONK HORNY XY WAXY KIX ATATA HORNY SWEET WAY | SVE’ Sx sx SX SX SX Fic. 1. Diagram showing the gametic composition of second-generation hybrids between waxy and sweet varieties of maize Since in both sweet and waxy the alternative factor necessary to produce horny is assumed to be lacking, the gametes produced by sweet varieties are represented by Sa and the gametes produced by varieties with waxy endo- sperm by sX. The synthetic horny produced by crossing waxy and sweet is then represented by a combination of 586 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vow. XLVIII these, or SxsX. Assuming a chance recombination of these factors in the gametes derived from these synthetic horny seeds, the gametes will be of four kinds. Both the sweet and the waxy may be present (SX) or the sweet may be present without the waxy (Sa), or the waxy with- out the sweet (sX), or both may be absent (sx). At ferti- lization each of these kinds of gametes may unite with any one of the four corresponding kinds derived from the other parent, producing 16 zygotic combinations. In the diagram the four classes of gametes from one parent are given in the horizontal row at the top, and the same four classes from the other parent in the vertical row at the left. Each gametic combination from the top is repeated four times in the squares below, while each combination at the side occurs four times in the corresponding hori- zontal row of squares. Thus each of the squares repre- sents the result obtained by combining the gametes repre- senting the horizontal and vertical rows that intersect at that point. In all cases where both S and X occur together the seed should be horny, where only S occours the-seed should be sweet, when only X occurs it.should be waxy, and in one square (No. 16), where neither S nor X occurs there is a new combination which the results have shown to be a new type of sweet seed, indistinguishable from ordinary sweet seed but behaving differently when crossed with other types of endosperm. In accordance with the above analysis the expected re- sults in the third xenia generation were as follows: Proportion- Proportions f Seed ate No. of Ears. Classes. Self-pollinated horny. i 1 All horny 2 3 horny: 1 sweet 2 3 horny: 1 waxy 4 9 horny: 4 sweet: 3 waxy Self-pollinated sweet. : All sweet Self-pollinated waxy. 1 All waxy 2 3 waxy: 1 sweet No. 574] INHERITANCE 587 Crosses between different plants eds. from horny se 25 All horny 20 3 horny: 1 sweet 20 3 horny: 1 waxy 16 9 horny: 4 sweet: 3 waxy Crosses between different plants from sweet seeds. All sweet Crosses between different plants from waxy seeds 5 All waxy 4 3 waxy: 1 sweet Crosses between horny and sweet. 3 All horny 6 1 horny: 1 sweet 1 1 horny: 1 waxy 2 3 horny: 1 waxy 2 1 horny: 2 sweet: 1 waxy ? 4 3 horny: 4 sweet: 1 waxy Crosses between horny and waxy. 5 All horn 4 3 horny: 1 sweet 10 1 horny: 1 waxy 8 3 horny: 2 sweet: 3 waxy Crosses between sweet and waxy. All horny All waxy Ae bo po bd HS HY bat i= : td 1 horny: 2 sweet: 1 waxy THIRD XENIA GENERATION Four of the ears bearing second xenia generation seed were selected for planting in 1913, one self- and one cross- pollinated ear from each of the two hybrid families Dh 216 and Dh 221. These families were selected because in 1913 the family Dh 221 showed the greatest deficiency of sweet seeds and Dh 216 was the only family that showed sweet seeds in excess of the expected. The three classes of seeds from each of the ears were planted separately. Unfortunately as the result of an accident crosses were not made between the plants grown from the different classes, but a total of 77 selfed ears were obtained, a num- 588 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII ber sufficient to indicate whether the initial assumption | regarding the gametic compositions was of value in arranging the observed facts. PROGENY or SWEET SEEDS Sweet seeds were assumed to result from squares 6, 8, 14 and 16. It will be seen that in none of these is there any factor other than S and since the absence of both factors, as in square 16, is also assumed to produce sweet, we should expect nothing but all sweet ears from self- pollinated plants grown from sweet seeds. Seventeen self-pollinated ears were secured from plants grown from sweet seeds. All the seeds of these ears were sweet with the exception of one waxy seed. This one waxy seed was colored and since it occurred on an ear from a white sweet seed that otherwise produced only white sweet seeds, the exception may reasonably be ascribed to accidental foreign pollen. Progeny or Waxy SEEDS Waxy seeds were assumed to have resulted from the combinations shown in squares 11, 12 and 15. Seeds from square 11 should produce only waxy seeds. Squares 12 and 15 should produce ears with waxy and sweet seeds in proportion of 3 waxy to 1 sweet. There should, there- fore, be one all waxy ear to two with both waxy and sweet seeds. There were in all 29 ears from waxy seeds, 11 of which were all waxy and 18 with both waxy and sweet seeds. The numbers are small but at least both kinds of ears were secured and the proportion does not violate the original assumption. The 18 ears with both waxy and sweet seeds all produced them in approximately the 3:1- ratio. The numbers are given in Table I. The totals with 3,154 seeds indicate that if there is a deviation, it is almost certainly less than 2 per cent. All the sweet seeds that occur on ears grown from waxy seeds are assumed to belong to the new class of sweet seeds corresponding to that represented in square 16. No. 574] INHERITANCE 589 Plantings of such seeds are being made for comparison with the ordinary class of sweet seeds having the same ancestry. These are represented by the sweet seeds occur- ring on ears having horny and sweet seeds. TABLE I WAXY SEEDS SELF-POLLINATED, EARS SHOWING WAXY AND SWEET SEEDS. EXPECTED: 25 PER CENT. SWEET P tE | Pedigree a rey | ho Bab | Per Cent. of | Devia- a r | n | is t hi okt ti | Number is | Secd, | Seeds | Sweet Seeds Ereb. 1938 | 301| 216| 85 | 28.241.7 | +1.9 1939 | 112] 85| 27 +27 |= 3 - | {|1940 | 264! 202) 62 | 23.5 +18 |- 8 aay eee {1942 | 18| 14| 4 | 222466 | — 14 | [1948 | 349| 258| 91 | 261416 | + .7 1949 | 149| 100 49 | 329426 +3.0 (1950 | 138| 103| 35 | 254425 | - 2 | {1972 | g0 87 | 224414 | -19 973 | 187| 136, 51 | 27.3422 | +1.0 othe | |1974 | 174| 138| 36 | 2072421 | —2.0 , | 4 1975 | | 18 | 212430 | -13 penal EE | [1976 | 34] 2t) 13 | 382456 | +24 “| $1977 | 813} 232]. 81 | 2692417 | + 5 | (1978 | 109|- 79| 30 | 29 + 9 | (1904 | 136| 105| 31 | 228424 | -— 9 Dh 221-2 | $1905 |. 155| 116| 39 | 25.2423 | + .1 (Self-Pollinated) | } 1996 51] 31) 2% | 39.7 £46 | +3.1 | (1997 190 | 146 | 44 | 22.2421 | — 9 | Total... 3,154 | 2,351, 803 255+ 5 +1.0 Progeny oF Horny SEEDS From the horny seeds the expected results are more complicated. They may be tabulated as follows: 1 ear (Square 1) with seeds all horny 2 ears (Squares 2 and 5) with seeds 3 horny: 1 sweet 2 ears (Squares 3 and 9) with seeds 3 horny: 1 w 4 ears (Squares 4, 7, 10 and 13) with seeds 9 horny: 4 sweet: 3 waxy. Ears were, therefore, expected in the proportion of 1 all horny ear, 2 with horny and sweet seeds, 2 with horny and waxy seeds and 4 with all three classes. Thirty ears were secured from seed classed as horny. These ears were distributed as follows: 1 all horny, 5 with horny and 590 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII sweet seeds, 3 with horny and waxy seeds, 19 with horny, sweet and waxy seeds and 2 all sweet. The two all sweet ears are entirely outside the expected. Their appearance may be explained on the assumption that seeds classed as horny in 1912 were in reality sweet. No microscopical examination of the starch was made and the seeds .were classified on their appearance, wrinkled seeds being classed as sweet and smooth seeds as horny. The separation of horny from sweet seeds is more difficult to make than waxy from either horny or sweet.’ There were, however, very few doubtful seeds in the second xenia generation and in suggesting this interpre- tation, we may with some propriety be accused of attempt- ing to explain away ‘‘green balls.’’ 3 The two all sweet ears were descendants of an ear Dh 221-2, which showed an excess of horny seeds and a deficiency of sweet. The expected number of sweet seeds in Dh 221-2, which had a total of 493 seeds, was 123 and only 106 were classified as sweet. If this deviation re- sulted from a faulty classification, that is, if some of the sweet seeds failed to show the characteristic wrinkled exterior, we might expect that about 17 of the 300 seeds classed as horny would produce ears with all sweet seeds. Eleven of the ears secured from horny seeds in 1913 were descendents of this ear. The remaining 28 ears from horny seeds are distributed among the 3 classes in reasonably close agreement to the expected. Measured by Pearson’s formula for the good- ness of fit, it appears that such a deviation might be expected once in about twenty times. 2The difficulty of distinguishing between sweet and starchy seeds in crosses where the starchy variety has small seeds has been pointed out by East and Hays, ‘‘Inheritance in Maize,’’ Bull. 167, Conn. Ag. Exp. Sta., 1911, p. 40. 3 Pearson, K., and Heron, D., ‘‘On Theories of Association,’’ Biometrika, IX, pp. 309-314, 4 Phil. Mag., Vol. L, 1900, pp. 157-175. The application of Pearson’s formula to data of this kind was called to our attention by Mr. G. Udney Yule. No. 574] INHERITANCE 591 The three ears with horny and waxy seeds produced these classes in the expected 3:1 ratio. The numbers are given in Table II. TABLE II Horny SEEDS SELF-POLLINATED. Ears SHOWING HORNY AND WAXY SEEDS. EXPECTED: 25 PER p ai, WAXY D Per Cent. of tion + : x Total R No | No. Pedigree | N ‘Horny | Waxy | Parent E arent Kar Number Moke | Beedle | Sead | Waxy Seeds Pian h 216-2 | | | (Self-Pollinated) 1962 | 327 | 247 | 80 | M6456 | 3 Dh 221-2 2000 | 312 | 285 | 77 | 24.7216 2 (Self-Pollinated) 2007 | 121 | 82 39 | 322429 25 Total...| 760 | 564 | 196 | 258411 | 7 Four of the five ears that produced horny and sweet seeds were also as close as could be expected to the 3:1 ratio. The fifth, however, Ped. 1965, with 249 seeds, had only 19 sweet seeds or 7.6 per cent. The numbers are given in Table III. The only explanation that can be TABLE III HORNY SEEDS SELF-POLLINATED. EARS SHOWING HORNY AND SWEET SEEDs. EXPECTED: 25 PER CENT. SWEET <= Devia- | Total | No. | No. Per Cent. of | Devia reer | ee ay ay | rr Bro | Dh 216-1 | (Cross-Pollinated) 1965 | 249) 230 | w 76 #L1 | 16.0 Dh 216-2 (Self-Pollinated) 1979 | 442| 344 | 98 | 222413 | 22 Dh 221-1 | (Cross-Pollinated) 1988 | 160| 121 39 | 244423 .3 221-2 2003 175 | 184 | ü | 28.442.1 8 (Self-Pollinated) 2008 179 141 38 21.2 + 2.1 1.8 Total. ..| 1,205 | 970 | 235 | 195+ 8 | 6.9 offered in connection with this exceptional ear is that suggested for the occurrence of the two all sweet ears among those grown from seeds classed as horny, namely, the existence of sweet seeds which failed to show a 592 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII wrinkled surface. This explanation is rendered less prob- able, however, by the unusual behavior of the aleurone color in this same ear. In the previous discussion the aleurone color has not been considered. To treat of the aleurone color would naturally lead to the question of correlation between that character and endosperm texture, a subject which in these crosses is very complicated and for the treatment of which the results thus far obtained are inadequate. It may be said, however, that with the exception of Ped. 1965 the proportions of colored to white seeds in all the ears bear out the assumption that the in- heritance of the aleurone color is governed by two factors, both of which must be present to produce color. In Ped. 1965, however, which was grown from a colored seed, only 23 of the 249 seeds were white. The colored and white seeds are beautifully distinct with no intermediate or doubtful seeds. The ratio of 9.2 per cent. white might be - explained as an approximation to the dihybrid ratio of 6.25 per cent. but we must then admit that instead of both factors being necessary for the development of color either factor alone may produce color. The 19 ears from horny seeds that showed all three classes are assumed to have the same gametic composi- tion as the original second xenia generation, previously reported. The numbers are given in Table IV. The last column of the table gives the odds in 1,000 that deviations equal to those observed are not chance deviations from the expected proportions, as calculated by Pearson’s formula. Thus in Pedigree 1953 the odds are 809 to 191, or practi- cally 4 to 1, that the deviation is not the result of chance. As in the original ears, the approximation is sufficiently close to render futile any attempt to predicate a different arrangement of factors, but many of the deviations are too large to be ascribed to chance. In the totals the sweet class is too low and the waxy too high, in fact there is no significant difference between the totals for these two classes. The deviation from the expected is, however, No. 574] INHERITANCE 593 largely the result of two ears Ped. 1954 and 1967, and if the explanation suggested for the two all sweet ears from horny seeds is admitted, it may also account for the devia- tion in these two ears. In both ears the deficiency of sweet seeds is accompanied by an excess of horny seeds, while in neither ear is there a significant excess of waxy seeds. TABLE IV HORNY SEEDS SELF-POLLINATED. EARS SHOWING ALL THREE CLASSES. EXPECTED; 56.25 PER CENT, Horny, 25 Ses CENT. SWEET 18.75 PER CENT. W i : nees Horny Seeds Sweet Seeds Waxy Seeds pe 1000 Pedi- | Total ` ‘ in evia Parent .Ear No. | g Seeds No. Ex-|No. Ob-|No. Ex-|No. Ob-|No. Ex-|No. Ob-| is not pected | served | pected | served pected | served | Fera | 197 | 236 87 36 66 | 78 999+ 1955) 198! 111| 103| 50 37 | 40 |470 1956 42| 17 | 11 | 13 16 |777 B gs hang 1957| 148) g3| 74| 37| 42 | 28| 32 Pollinated) ) 1958| -176| , 99| 100 41 35 |133 963) 540 289| 135 | 133 | 101 | 118 | 832 1 70 4| 18 13 | 18 | 983 1966 170 101| 42 | 35 | 32 | 34 | 524 1967, 158. s9| 108| 39 | 23 | 30 27 |995 Dh 216-2 | Pollinated)| 1980 35; 20| 16| 9 |. 12 7 | 8 |486 Dh 221-1 1985) 258| 145 131| 65 73 | 48 | 54 |784 Cra. 1986, 120) 67| 72, 30| 25 | 23| 23 |443 Pollinated) | 1987) 375 211| 201| 94 | 92 | 70 82 |716 1909] 77) 43| 42| w] 16 | .14 | 19 |676 Dh 221-2 oh oF well grown is a dessert apple of fine appearance and high quality. In many of its characters, but especially in flavor and aroma, Delicious resembles the White Pearmain, though in color it bears a likeness to the Winesap group. In moderately elevated situations in some parts of the state it displays a well-balanced adaptation and attains excellent size, color, texture and quality, though none of those examined were quite equal in quality to the Deli- cious from New York. When grown in too low and warm a location the fruit has a tendency to become overripe and when stored tends to soften in the center, after which it loses greatly in quality. The sample from Clarkston had 610 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII a beautiful dark red color, while that from Cloverland was dull in color and poor in texture. Esopus (Spitzenburg).—This is almost the only variety which the writer has examined that attains the first rank as a dessert apple in this state. In certain sections it dis- plays a better balance of adaptation so far as flesh charac- ters are concerned than any other variety. The samples obtained from White Salmon and the irrigated valleys were of excellent quality as dessert apples, though of scarcely as good texture as the variety attains in the east. Overgrown apples are especially coarse in texture. West of the Cascades and in the more elevated locations the Esopus does not reach as high quality as elsewhere. This variety is inclined to wilt in storage unless well grown. Gano.—This is an apple of the Ben Davis type, but of a ‘more uniform red color. Practically all the remarks in- cluded under Ben Davis, aside from those dealing with the distribution of color, apply equally well to the Gano. At its best, the Gano is of slightly better quality than the Ben Davis, which fact, together with its more handsome ap- pearance, renders it a more desirable variety to plant, yet neither can be recommended in a section desirous of build- ing up a reputation and market for dessert apples. It is interesting that both the highest color and the best as well as the poorest quality was attained by apples from the east and middle west. Grimes (Grimes Golden).—This variety, like the Ben Davis, displays considerable variation in form, depending on the locality of production. The specimens from the middle west were roundish to decidedly oblate, while those grown in Washington were all more or less elongated. Those grown west of the Cascades displayed a greater tendency to a conical shape than those from the eastern part of the state, and were also poorer in quality. When grown in the more elevated sections, as at Pullman, Grimes appears poorly developed and immature and is inferior in size and quality. Those from Grandview dis- played the best balance of characters and it seems prob- No. 574] VARIATION IN THE APPLE 611 able that this variety is better adapted to the irrigated valleys than to other sections of the state. All samples were more or less wilted by midwinter, except the fruit from Grandview, which remained firm but showed some tendency to rot. Scald was very bad in the latter part of the season. Jonathan.—Although rather extensively grown in a number of localities in Washington, none of the fruit which the writer has examined gave evidence of a well- balanced adaptation to the conditions of growth which prevail in the state. All were inferior in color to the fruit _ obtained from the east and middle west. The apples from Clarkston and the Yakima Valley were of good size but lacked both richness of flavor and aroma. The same lack was evident in the fruit from the western part of the state. At Pullman a pretty good quality is attained, but the _ fruit does not come up to the requirements as to size and gives other evidence of imperfect development. At Cloverland and in other elevated locations fruit of a poor texture and deficient coloring is produced. Jonathan seems to reach its highest development in certain sections tributary to the Ohio valley and the Washington-grown Jonathans can not compete with fruit from that section when well grown. The samples from Morgantown, West Virginia, were of a beautiful clear dark red color, good size, fine tender flesh, and very high quality. In storage these specimens remained firm and retained their flavor until April. The others wilted considerably after mid- winter. Lawver.—tThis variety attains good size and fine color ‘in the irrigated valleys, but the quality is not good enough to recommend it to the fruit growers of the northwest. The variety ordinarily keeps well but the specimens stored proved to have poor keeping quality—owing to fungous infection. McIntosh_—The McIntosh is deserving of attention as a variety of high quality which appears to have a fairly well-balanced adaptation to certain sections of the north- 612 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII west. At Pullman the elevation is too great for the best development of the variety, but the Spokane Valley pro- ~ duces MeIntoshes of a high degree of excellence. There is good reason to believe that the valley of the northern ` and northeastern sections of the state can rival the Bitter Root valley of Montana in the production of this variety. The fruit stored wilted badly by midwinter and lost much of its flavor soon after. Northern Spy.—Of all the varieties examined the Northern Spy seems least adapted to the conditions of growth in this state. As produced in New York and New England this fruit is a dessert apple of the highest quality when well grown and properly colored. In Washington east of the Cascades the color fails to develop and the quality is much inferior to that of the eastern-grown fruit. In the western part of the state the color develops as well as in the eastern states, but the quality is no better than . elsewhere in the state. The unsurpassed cooking quality of this variety seems to be largely retained, however, which is its only redeeming feature. It may be worth planting to a limited extent as a culinary fruit for home use, but can not compete in the markets with the eastern- grown Northern Spys. The specimens from the western part of the state were largely infected with fungi, result- ing in much decay early in the season. Those from Pull- man and Clarkston kept fairly well, though the former wilted badly late in the season. Rhode Island Greening.—This variety, together with Baldwin and Northern Spy, constitutes the most promi- nent and successful apples in the orchards of New York and New England. They are also among the varieties least’ adapted to the conditions found in this state. Their per- fect balance of adaptation to eastern conditions is prob- ably to a large degree responsible for their popularity in the east and may also account for the lack of balance which they display in the northwest. As grown at White Salmon and at Pullman the Greening reached a good size, but was decidedly inferior in quality to the specimens No. 574] VARIATION IN THE APPLE 613 from New Hampshire. At Pullman the fruit was rather flat and strongly ribbed, while at White Salmon the apples were oblong in shape and had, as a rule, rather small cavities. It can not be recommended for Washington, except possibly for local use as a culinary fruit. This variety is a fairly good keeper. Those grown at Pullman wilted badly late in the season, while the lot from White Salmon gave evidence of considerable fungus infection. Rome (Rome Beauty).—This is one of the most popu- lar varieties grown in the state east of the Cascade Moun- tains and is about the only commercial variety which reaches good marketable size in the high uplands of the Inland Empire. The Rome reaches its highest develop- ment in the Jonathan belt of the middle west. The best specimens examined, all characters considered, came from Morgantown, West Virginia. They were of a nearly uni- form deep red color, of good size and attractive form, and of pretty good quality for the variety. In many parts of Washington the Rome fails to color well. The specimens from White Salmon and Grandview were especially poor in color. The latter were overgrown and of poor quality, while the former were among the best of the variety. The usual form of the variety is round or nearly so, varying to somewhat roundish conic or roundish ovate. The form of the cavity is subject to quite a little variation. As pro- duced at Pullman and other elevated sections of the state the cavity is very shallow, but becomes deeper in the val- leys. The specimens from West Virginia had fairly deep cavities. Indeed it seems probable that those localities which produce Ben Davis of the elongated type also pro- duce Romes with the shallow cavities. The Rome is by nature a culinary apple. In quality it is but little better than Ben Davis. It seems unfortunate, therefore, for the lasting reputation of the industry, that it should have be- come so firmly established in northwestern horticulture. It is to be earnestly hoped that it may in time be replaced by a variety of better quality. In its adaptations to the conditions of the state, the Rome seems to be fairly well 614 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII balanced in most of its characters. The balance, however, is not the same in all sections and is nowhere quite so per- fect as in certain localities in the middle states. Most samples kept well until the latter part of the season and then became mealy. The overgrown specimens from Grandview were the first to break down in this way. Those grown at a greater elevation showed a slight tend- ency to wilt late in the season. None of the samples dis- played an inclination to rot until late in the season. Stayman Winesap.—In both size and quality the Stay- man is the best of the Winesap group. Its most serious fault is a rather dull color which often fails to cover the fruit well. The samples obtained from the middle west were of better color and texture than those grown in Washington, though the lot from Indiana were very coarse in texture. Those grown at Pullman were small and in- ferior in every way. The fruit from Grandview was especially large, flat, and fairly well colored, while that from White Salmon was more elongated, slightly less colored, and rather more aromatic in flavor. These two lots retained their firmness in storage much longer than the others and those from White Salmon scalded badly late in the season. It is very similar to the Winesap in its adaptations. Tompkins King.—This variety is popular in the west- ern part of the state, where it attains a large size and good color, though the latter character develops well at Pull- man. None of the samples equaled in quality the variety as grown in New York. Those grown at Pullman had a very good flavor, though the flesh characters were those of poorly matured fruit. The fruit from the western part of the state was of a fairly elongated conic form, while that grown at Pullman was shorter and strongly ribbed. This variety appears to be but poorly adapted to Washington conditions. The fruit grown at Pullman wilted badly late in the season, while that from western Washington rotted considerably owing to fungus infections. Wagener—Though of the Northern Spy class, the No. 574] VARIATION IN THE APPLE 615 Wagener displays a much better balance of adaptation to the conditions of the state than the Northern Spy. It seems to reach its best development in the cooler regions of the state. The specimens from Grandview were of good size and very juicy, but were poor in color, coarse in texture, and deficient in flavor. Wagener develops especially well in the Spokane Valley. The specimens from Opportunity were large, well colored, and of excel- lent quality, though somewhat coarse in texture. Those grown at Pullman were more aromatic but possibly not so rich in flavor and did not develop sufficient size. This variety does well west of the Cascades and especially in the northern part of the Puget Sound Basin. The speci- mens from Eastsound were large, highly colored, and fine in texture, but less aromatic than the eastern Washington fruit. The samples obtained from West Virginia gave evidence of having been grown too far south. They were poorly colored and of rather poor texture, but of good size and excellent flavor. In form the fruit from Opportunity was roundish, that from Eastsound roundish conic, while the remainder was decidedly flattened and all samples were more or less strongly ribbed. This variety shows very little tendency to wilt in storage. The fruit from the highlands keeps well, but that from the irrigated valleys shows a tendency to physiological decay. Scald is serious after midwinter. ` White Pearmain (White Winter Pearmain).—In gen- eral appearance this variety often closely resembles the Yellow Newtown, but is usually more elongated and more largely blushed. Moreover, it is quite different in flavor and is remarkable for its fine aroma. It is a variety of high quality and attractive for a yellow apple, moreover, it attains its good qualities in the irrigated valleys better than on the highlands, the specimens from Cloverland be- ing dull and green in color and poor in texture, but well blushed and highly aromatic. Its worst fault is suscepti- bility to the apple scab. It would seem to be better adapted to growing in the state than some of the more 616 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII popular varieties. The fruit from the Yakima Valley retained its firmness much better than that from Clover- land, but lost somewhat in flavor toward the close of the season. Willow (Willow-Twig).—The writer has examined this variety only as grown in the elevated portions of eastern Washington. In such locations it does not develop espe- cially well in either size or color and is of too poor quality to be worthy of consideration. Moreover, it wilts badly in storage, though when well grown the fruit has excellent keeping quality. It is evidently poorly adapted to this section. Winesap.—In some of the irrigated valleys this variety is one of the most popular apples grown. It attains a good marketable size and an attractive color, though none of the samples examined were equal in color or quality to the Winesaps from Indiana and West Virginia. In ele- vated localities, as at Pullman, Cloverland and White Salmon, the fruit is small and poorly colored and has flesh characters indicating imperfect development and matur- ity. As grown in the irrigated valleys the fruit is apt to be deficient in flavor, and, if large, coarse in texture. The lot from Cashmere showed the best balance of characters of any Washington, grown specimens, but these were in no way superior to the Winesaps from West Virginia. It is probable that the better grown fruit from the eastern Winesap districts is equal to that grown in Washington in all respects, with the possible exception of size, which, if large, is, as noted, apt to be accompanied by deterioration in quality. It is evident then, that the balance of adapta- tion of this variety to northwestern conditions is imper- fect at best and that the planting of Winesaps in Wash- ington may easily be overdone. This variety proved to be one of the best in keeping quality. Those from Pullman and Cloverland wilted late in the season, though most of the other lots were in excellent condition in April and a few were held in storage until July. Winter Banana.—As only a single lot of this variety No. 574] VARIATION IN THE APPLE 617 was examined in detail, it is difficult to make very positive statements regarding its behavior in the state. Though less desirable than a red apple, it is a variety of handsome appearance and is fairly good in quality. It is perhaps rather better adapted than the averageto certain sections of the state and appears to develop best in fairly elevated situations. It is especially well liked in the Spokane Val- ley, and fruit grown there is said to have good keeping quality, though the specimens from western Washington were past season by midwinter. They wilted badly and showed much scald. Yellow Bellflower—This variety appears to be better adapted to the western part of the state than to the irri- gated valleys. The apples from Clarkston were coarser in texture, milder in flavor and poorer in quality than the samples received from the east. There were no very striking differences in form, structure or appearance ex- cept that the eastern Bellflowers were more often blushed than those from Clarkston. The apples from Puyallup were overgrown specimens from young trees, were coarse and spongy in texture, and inferior in quality. As this is a tender fruit, easily injured by careless handling, and does not appear to be especially well balanced in its adaptations, it is not desirable to plant extensively for shipping. Moreover, it is not a good keeper. The speci- mens from Puyallup were practically past season when received and those obtained from the east were more or less injured and such specimens decayed quickly. Some of the lot from Clarkston, however, kept sound and firm until past midwinter, but deteriorated in flavor toward the last. Yellow Newtown.—When at its best, this variety has few equals. It is narrow in the limits of its adaptations and its successful culture in the eastern states is confined to small areas, where, however, it is in nearly perfect equilibrium with its environment. In many places in the northwest it is grown successfully, though it scarcely equals in quality the best eastern product. The fruit from White Salmon and some of the irrigated districts 618 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVII was of excellent quality, but coarser and less delicate in texture and of not quite so good flavor as the apples from West Virginia. The specimens from Cloverland were hard and green and gave evidence of imperfect maturity. Evidently the elevation is too great for its proper devel- opment. The single sample from western Washington consisted of well-colored, extensively blushed fruit, but was inferior in quality. Owing to its limited area of suc- cessful production in the east, it is worth planting in Washington wherever its characters give evidence of a fair degree of balance of adaptation with the environ- ment. This variety is perhaps a better keeper than Wine- sap. Some of the fruit from White Salmon kept in good condition until July, though overgrown fruit and that which has been exposed to heat before storage showed signs of physiological decay late in the season. Under- developed specimens wilted in storage. York Imperial_—In sections of Virginia and neighbor- ing states the York Imperial occupies the place of su- premacy held by the Baldwin farther north. This is doubtless due to its perfect balance with the environmental conditions of that region, and, like the Baldwin and other sorts perfectly adapted to their eastern habitat, this variety finds itself out of equilibrium when moved to the northwest. The apples from western Washington were of good size and color, but were coarse and undesirable in texture and poor in quality. The specimens grown at Pullman were smaller, more elongated, and less com- pressed than the others, and the axes were less oblique. They were somewhat better in quality, though not good enough to justify more extensive planting. The fruit wilted in storage, and that from western Washington gave evidence of fungous infection and scalded badly after midwinter. DISCUSSION OF THE EFFECT OF ENVIRONMENT Upon APPLE CHARACTERS Size—Size is the direct result of development. An apple will reach its maximum in growth when all factors No. 574] VARIATION IN THE APPLE 619 are at the variety optimum for the physiological proc- esses upon which development depends. A departure from this optimum, whether toward a greater or less in- tensity, means a decrease in size, as is observed in ap- proaching either the northern or southern range of a variety. It has been frequently noted, however, that the optimum for growth is not the best combination of fact- ors for the development of certain other desirable char- acters, so that it is well to choose an environment having certain factors in a somewhat less degree of intensity, being content with fruit of fair size but superior in other respects. Since the apple contains about 85 per cent. of moisture it is evident that the water supply is a factor of prime importance in determining size. It is possible by excessive irrigation to force an abnormal growth of the fruit, though always apparently at the expense of text- ure, flavor, and keeping quality. It is evident, then, that if fruit of good quality is expected, irrigation must be moderate in amount, especially with vigorous young trees. Thinning may result in increased size owing to the larger amount of moisture available for each fruit. Temperature and length of season are of importance in determining, respectively, the rapidity of growth and de- gree of development attained. Form.—One of the striking features revealed by the ~ study of a number of varieties from several localities is the fact that the modification in shape due to the differ- ence in environment is by no means uniform for the several varieties. Some varieties are quite constant in shape while others are much more plastic in this respect. Moreover, certain varieties are much more easily in- fluenced than others which respond in the same way, while still others respond differently to the same factors. One of the most frequently observed and conspicuous modifications of form consists of the elongation of the axis of the fruit relative to the horizontal diameter. This character has been especially studied, in the case of the Ben Davis, by Shaw, who found the elongation most noticeable in fruit from the northeastern states, the mari- 620 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vov. XLVIII time provinces of Canada, and the Pacific coast. Shaw’s papers dealing with this subject have already been noted. Upon studying the climate in these localities, it was found that the temperature for two or three weeks after the blooming season was notably lower than in the sections where the Ben Davis assumes its normal shape. Since this appeared to be the only factor constant for the several localities, it is suggested as the explanation of this variation. It has been shown, however, that temper- ature is incapable of influencing form except by its action through the functions of the plant in modifying the effect of some other factor. It is the writer’s opinion that the elongation is due to the relative moisture supply of the different parts of the apple at this period of develop- ment as influenced by the temperature ; that it is primar- ily a modification due to the moisture relation rather than to the direct effect of temperature, the latter being a secondary cause. The rapidity of circulation of the sap and therefore the supply of moisture to the organs of the plant is greatly influenced by the temperature. It is a well-known fact of plant physiology that much less moist- ure passes through the plant in the cool days of spring than during the warmer weather of midsummer. A re- duction of the temperature at this time results in a still more sluggish movement of the sap. In the period im- mediately after blooming the energy of the plant, so far as the development of the fruit is concerned, is directed primarily to the proper nourishment of the growing seeds and the adjacent parts. If at this time the circulation of the sap is retarded by a temperature unwontedly low for the variety, the moisture supply of the fruit is lessened and a relatively larger amount goes to the seeds and adjacent parts, while the pulpy portion of the fruit receives a more scant supply. As a result, the axillary development is proportionately greater than the swelling of the fruit due to the accumulation of moisture in the superficial tissues. After some two or three weeks the form of the fruit becomes fixed and is not noticably in- fluenced by the moisture supply thereafter. No. 574] VARIATION IN THE APPLE 621 The elongation of the fruit is usually accompanied by a constriction of the apex resulting in a conical form. This may be due to the greater development of the basal portion, which is adjacent to the point where the sap enters the fruit and may therefore be better supplied, though the physiology of fruit development is in need of further study. In the Grimes, however, an oblong form results. The McIntosh, as grown at Pullman, is often decidedly obovate, a variation which the writer ascribes to the same influences that produce the elongated conic form of the Ben Davis and other varieties, though in this . variety the response is somewhat different. The Rhode Island Greening, Willow and Wagener, as a rule, fail to assume an elongated form in localities where it is well marked in some other varieties. Also in certain varie- ties which are naturally conic in form and considerably elongated, as Delicious and Yellow Bellflower, this effect is not evident. The larger number of varieties, when grown in this state, have a more ribbed form than the same varieties in the east. This seems to be due to a lack of balance in adaptation, though the particular factor which gives rise to the variation has not been determined. Some varieties, like the York Imperial and the Yellow Newtown, are compressed in form, that is elliptical in section, and have an oblique axis when grown in certain environments. These characters seem to be in some way related to the better development of the fruit, as they are less evident in fruit from the elevated and unfavorable sections of the state. Beach has noted in the ‘‘ Apples of New York’’ a similar difference between the Newtowns of western New York and those of the Hudson Valley, the latter having a more oblique axis and elliptical form. Stem.—The stem is one of the most variable structures of the apple, and, owing to the fact that stems of different lengths, diameters and shapes are commonly found in any lot of apples grown under practically uniform con- ditions, it is difficult to associate such variations with the environment. The writer has noted, however, in the case of some short-stemmed varieties, like the York Imperial, 622 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII that those lots grown under less favorable conditions had, on the average, longer stems than others grown under a more favorable environment. Cavity.—The most conspicuous variation in the cavity is inits depth. This is of especial note in the Rome, which has a very shallow cavity in most parts of the state. This is doubtless due to the same cause which produces the elongated form of the fruit in many varieties, namely the elongation of the axis resulting from a deficient moisture supply incident to a low temperature after the blooming season. In this variety the elongated axis obliterates the cavity instead of modifying the general outline of the fruit. The same variation is also noted to a less degree in a number of other varieties. An especially furrowed cavity is often observed associated as a rule with the ribbed form of fruit. Calyx.—The writer has failed to observe any modifica- tions of importance in the calyx lobes of the fruit. The size of the calyx cup or ‘‘ eye ”’ of the apple is influenced by the development of the fruit. In large fruit this open- ing is apt to be large, so that the lobes are separated, re- sulting in an open or partly open calyx. Small or poorly developed apples, on the other hand, usually have the calyx closed. Basin.—The depth of the basin seems to depend upon the same factors as that of the cavity and seems to be much more readily influenced than the latter. The width is often associated with the form of the apple, a very con- ° stricted apex resulting in a narrow basin. A much fur- rowed basin results from a combination of factors un- favorable to the best development of the fruit. Skin Statements have often appeared in regard to the effect of various climatic factors upon the thickness and toughness of the skin. Estimates of these characters, however, appear to be based entirely upon sense impres- sions of the observers, although it would seem that exact measurements would not be especially difficult. In the absence of such accurate data, an expression of opinion he No. 574] VARIATION IN THE APPLE 623 would be premature. Dry air and sunshine are favorable to the production of clear, smooth skin. Color.—There seems to be no doubt that the coloration of apples depends upon the influence of several factors of which light is usually the most important. The impor- tance of light is easily demonstrated by covering the fruit during development either wholly or in part. The in- tensity of illumination is also, evidently, quite narrow in its limits, so that a point is soon reached at which the color begins to pale owing to excess of illumination. It has been frequently noted that apples grown near the southern limit of the range of a variety are paler than those grown farther to the north. This effect appears to be the result of an excess of the two factors, heat and light. It has been mentioned in the discussion of the characters of several varieties that, contrary to the gen- eral impression, those grown in Washington east of the Cascades where insolation is intense were less highly colored than those from western Washington or the east- ern states. The most marked example of this kind which the writer has observed is the Northern Spy. Again, contrary to the general impression, most of the samples from elevated locations were poorly colored, a fact which may be attributed partly to the strong insolation and partly to the poor development due to the low summer temperature. It appears, therefore, that either too strong or too weak illumination may result in poorly colored fruit and that the best color is developed under a condition of optimum intensity of the light. It is suggested above that temperature may influ- ence color. This is most commonly observed in the case of apples grown under conditions of too high summer temperature, though a deterioration in color also results if the temperature is much below the optimum for the variety. It is often stated that apples become more highly colored the farther north they are grown. This is only true in part. Those varieties which are adapted to the most northerly por- tions of the apple belt are able to develop their highest 624 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII color at the limit of winter hardiness of the tree. The southern varieties, on the other hand, require for the best development of color a higher summer temperature than is experienced in the northern localities. The Winesap, for example, when grown in Central New York is partly covered with a pale red. At Pullman the majority of varieties color poorly, due at least in part to the cool climate. That the temperature and not the shortness of the season is the factor involved is shown by the fact that most of these varieties color well in central New York which has a season of about the same length though averaging several degrees warmer. Cultural conditions may influence the color to a cer- tain degree. In general those processes of orchard man- agement which favor the early maturity of the fruit re- sult in improved color, especially in localities having a short growing season. Pruning and wide planting are regarded as favoring high coloration by admitting light into the tree, though it is possible that in regions where the light is intense these factors may not be of so great importance in their effect upon color as in less sunny loca- tions. Sométhing has been said of the influence of the soil in the discussion of the literature and it has been noted also that studies of the effect of fertilizers upon the color have not yielded satisfactory or uniform results. The influence of iron compounds is worthy of brief dis- cussion in this connection. It seems evident, from the chemical studies which have been made, that the red pig- ment includes iron in its composition. This has some- times been assumed to mean. that the chief requirement for highly colored fruit is the presence of plenty of avail- able iron compounds in the soil. As a matter of fact, iron is also necessary to the formation of chlorophyll and most soils contain an abundance of that element for the purpose. From the chemical data compiled by Stewart® it appears that the ash of the fruit contains a much smaller proportion of iron than that of the leaves. Itis | logical to conclude, therefore, that soils containing suff- 5 Pa. Sta. Rept. for 1910-11, No. 574] VARIATION IN THE APPLE ' 625 cient iron for the development of chlorophyll in the leaves are also fully supplied for the formation of the red pig- ment of the apple. Internal Structure. —The form and relative develop- ment of the core and associated structures are subject to numerous variations, which, however, are seldom so con- spicuous as to attract attention unless closely studied, and appear to be of little practical importance to either the grower or consumer of the fruit. The number of seeds may be mentioned as an indication of the thorough- ness of cross pollination and in most varieties the pres- ence of one or more well developed seeds is a requisite to the proper development of the fruit. Small or poorly developed fruit, the result of too short a season or too low a temperature, is apt to have the core closed and axile, or nearly so, while in the same varieties good develop- ment is usually associated with a more open abaxile core. The carpels of such poorly developed fruit are usually entire and smooth, while those of the better-grown fruit are more or less cleft and often tufted. Flesh Characters.—From the standpoint of the con- sumer, these are by all odds the most important charac- ters of the fruit, though lost sight of through the empha- sis placed on external characters, and no grower who has at heart the permanent prosperity, extension and normal development of the industry can afford to look upon quality as a secondary consideration. Neglect in this matter is sure to result sooner or later in a bad repu- tation for the fruit among a considerable proportion of buyers, which appearance and advertising will not be competent to overcome. The fact can not be denied that the great majority of varieties fail to attain as high quality in the northwest as when grown in the eastern or middle states where nearly all of them originated, while at the same time they may excel in other important char- acters. This is especially true of most of the choice dessert apples. Such unequal development can have no other interpretation than that these varieties are in a ` state of unbalanced adaptation to the environment. This 626 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII fact being recognized, the main question is, How can this disadvantage be overcome? Evidently the solution does not consist in a steadfast refusal to face the situation and vehement declaration that the fruit of any particular district is the best that can be produced. Such tactics, though well meant, can be permanently successful only when the statements are justified by the facts. If apple culture in Washington is to be maintained upon a sound basis it will be necessary first of all that growers shall exercise great care in planting to choose those varieties most nearly in equilibrium with the environment in the various sections of the state, at the same time avoiding over-irrigation or other errors in orchard management which may tend to an unequal development of the char- acters of the fruit, usually at the expense of quality. Even this, however, may be but a temporary makeshift, since few if any of the better varieties possess the re- quisite power of adaptation. It will be necessary first of all to determine if the variations which appear when apples are grown from seed in the northwest are more favorable in character than those which are displayed by introduced varieties. If such should prove to be the case the writer is under the conviction that the apple culture of the northwest should ultimately be largely made over on a basis of new varieties of local origin. A number of such varieties have already appeared, but unfortunately some of them have been chosen with little regard for quality. No work of greater value to the future horti- culture of the region can be undertaken by the experiment stations of the northwestern states than the development of apple varieties of high quality and perfect adaptation to the various sections of their respective states. The apples of high quality which show a fair degree of adaptation to the irrigated sections are Esopus, Yellow Newtown, Delicious and White Pearmain. The last was found by Lewis, of the Oregon station, to be one of the best pollenizers on every variety tested. Jonathan, Winesap and Stayman, though largely grown, shows in general a poorer balance of characters. In the more No. 574] VARIATION IN THE APPLE 627 elevated valleys Wagener, Delicious and McIntosh are doubtless most worthy of culture. The highlands of eastern Washington are very poorly adapted to the grow- ing of winter apples, though some of the early apples do fairly well, among which may be mentioned Oldenburg, Gravenstein and Yellow Transparent. On account of the abundance of sunshine the Oldenburg develops a high sugar content for the variety which counteracts its natural acidity and results in an apple of pretty good dessert quality. Of the winter apples, Rome reaches good marketable size but the quality is not high and the east- ern market should not be jeopardized by shipping this variety. The Palouse, an apple of local origin, is of much better quality, but has little standing in the market as yet. The Dutch Migonne, a variety from western Europe, shows a better balance of characters in eastern Washington than in most other sections of this country. It is of good size, fairly well colored and excellent in quality. Many varieties popular in the eastern states color better west of the Cascades than in eastern Washington, though there is usually manifest a lack of balance in other characters. In certain respects the environment resembles that of western Europe and many of the va- rieties of cherries, plums, prunes, and other fruits of that country do very well here and, indeed, in other sec- tions of the state as well, though in a number of instances varieties of northwestern origin are gaining in favor rapidly. Apple breeding, however, requires nfore time for its accomplishment and further importations of fruits, especially apples, adapted to the mild climate of western Europe would no doubt prove an advantage through the possible discovery of sorts adapted espe- cially to the western part of the state. ? Quality is not in itself a simple character. It depends upon all the characters of the flesh which determine the desirability of the fruit for eating, such as texture, juici- ness, aroma and flavor. Fineness of texture evidently depends upon a proper combination of favorable factors. 628 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII Conditions favoring rank growth result in coarse texture, as was observed in several instances in the case of apples grown under irrigation, especially if the fruit was over- grown. Some of the fruit from young trees also was overgrown and coarse. Tenderness depends upon the de- velopment. Poorly grown, under-developed fruit grown where the temperature is too low or the season too short for the variety has hard flesh which becomes spongy rather than mellow toward the end of the storage season. Overgrown fruit of certain varieties, on the other hand, often shows lack of coherence between the cells, often ac- companied apparently by larger intercellular spaces, and such fruit tends to become mealy as the season pro- gresses. Juiciness is primarily a manifestation of the amount of moisture in the fruit, but is also associated with the tenderness of the cell walls and their tendency to break rather than to separate. In general an abun- dance of moisture results in juicy fruit though the juici- ness is not in proportion to the moisture supply. The sub- stances which give the apple its aroma are present in such small amounts that their investigation is difficult. They are volatile compounds and affect the flavor of the apple largely by their action on the sense of smell. A cool climate is favorable to their production and it was often observed that they were most strongly developed in the apples from elevated situations. Flavor depends upon the kinds, amounts and relative proportions of the soluble solids, especially the balance between sugars and acids, antl will be given further consideration in the dis- cussion of the chemical composition. Immature and under-developed apples contain some tannic acid, which is often sufficient in amount to give an astringent charac- ter to the fruit. Keeping Quality.—In its relation to the environment, keeping quality evidently follows the same rule as other variable characters of the apple, namely, that for any variety the keeping quality depends upon the optimum intensity of the various external factors. Apples grown where the temperature is too low or the season too short No. 574] VARIATION IN THE APPLE . 629 to develop the fruit to a proper stage to keep well, soon wilt, lose flavor and scald, or show other evidence of de- terioration as was frequently observed in the fruit from high altitudes. On the other hand, too great excess of certain factors results in overgrown or overripe fruit having a tendency to rot, mealiness, or physiological de- cay, as in the case of the Yellow Bellflowers from Puyal- lup and some of the fruit from the warm valleys. - The balance of factors favorable to good keeping quality does not appear to differ much from that which produces the fruit which is most desirable in other characters, though it is possible that the required intensity of some factors may be slightly lower. It appears, therefore, that a good balance of the other characters of the fruit and perfect adaptation to the environment will be accompanied, as a rule, by good keeping quality, provided that the fruit is properly handled and not infected with disease, while an unbalanced adaptation of characters to environment is: likely to result in poor keeping quality. It seems prob- able that. irrigation in itself does not result in poor keep- ing except when improperly applied or carried to excess: or associated with other factors in such a way as to de- stroy the equilibrium of the environment. The relation: of specific gravity to the keeping quality is discussed in a succeeding paragraph. Specific Gravity—It has long been understood that varieties of apples differ in their relative weights; thus Wolf River is comparatively light and Baldwin is gener- ally regarded as a heavy apple. The only record found of the determination of specific gravity of apples is that of Howard’s work in the National Bureau of Chemistry, Bulletin 94, in which it is noted that the specifie gravity diminished 3 per cent. to 5 per cent. during storage. From the account it is not clear whether the determina- tions at the different dates were made with the same apples. The decrease of specific gravity is ascribed to the increase of air spaces between the cells due to the soften- ing of the middle lamella. In the specific gravity determi- nations made by the writer a number of points was noted. 630 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII The different lots of a variety may differ considerably in specific gravity, though as a rule running somewhat close together, thus Ben Davis and Gano are apples of low spe- cific gravity, while Grimes, Stayman, Wagener, and Yellow Newtown run rather high and Baldwin and Rome may be classed as medium in this respect. Overgrown apples were low in specific gravity, probably owing to more air space between the cells. This is more apparent upon examining the results for individual apples than upon comparing the average for different lots, as in the latter case the extremes are modified by averaging with the re- sults for more normal specimens. On the other hand, small and rather undeveloped apples are apt to have a high specific gravity on account of their solid flesh and usually closed core. Juicy apples, if not overgrown, have a high specific gravity when the juiciness is due to a high moisture content. The relation of specific gravity to keeping quality is of interest. While some late keeping varieties have nor- mally a low specific gravity, those lots of a given variety having a high specific gravity for the variety are usually the best keepers. This is in line with the fact that certain causes which give rise to fruit of poor keeping quality also produce a low specific gravity. This is shown very strikingly by a comparison of the specific gravities as cal- culated month by month through the season. As the ealeulations were made at the time the fruit was found fit for use, the monthly averages show the steady increase in specific gravity with the better keeping quality of the fruit, though modified somewhat by the peculiarities of the different varieties which happened to be in season at different times. These averages are as follows: Novem- ber and December, 0.787; January, 0.787; February, 0.810; March, 0.831; April, 0.852. Though these results may seem to be at variance with Howard’s observations it is possible that if the same specimens had been tested at intervals a decrease in specific gravity would have been noted. Chemical Composition.—In order to throw some light, No. 574] VARIATION IN THE APPLE 631 if possible, upon the relation of chemical composition to the other characters of the apple and to determine whether the composition is influenced by the environment, the juice of the various samples of Grimes, Jonathan, Winesap and Yellow Bellflower was analyzed by the de- partment of chemistry. The juice of the Grimes and Winesap contains, as a rule, a decidedly higher percentage of total solids than that of the Jonathan and Yellow Bellflower. It is also generally higher in specific gravity and has a greater viscosity. In Grimes and Yellow Bellflower the juice of the eastern-grown fruit contains a large proportion of total solids than that of the Washington grown fruit, though this rule does not hold good in the other varieties. The apples from the irrigated valleys and western Wash- ington were low in total solids with the single exception of the Winesaps from Cashmere. The analyses fail to show any constant difference in sugar content in favor of the fruit produced in the sunny climate with long hours of daylight characteristic of the apple-growing sections of the state. In Grimes the total sugars are fairly high and the pro- portion of sucrose is especially large. The acid content, on the other hand, is low as a rule. The result is a rich, mild or nearly sweet flavor. A sample from Puyallup showed the lowest sucrose content combined with the highest acid content, and this was the least rich as well as the most acid in flavor. Jonathan, on the other hand, displays a low content of total sugars and especially sucrose, while the acid content is slightly higher than in Grimes, indicating a subacid apple, lacking in richness. The lots from Missouri and Indiana were highest in sucrose but were of scarcely as good quality as the Jonathans from West Virginia. The latter were low in both sucrose and acid, but displayed a good balance between these constituents, indicating an apple with rather thin juice, not very rich, but pleasant and refreshing. Its evident superiority resulted largely 632 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII from the fine texture and well-developed flavoring con- stituents not shown by the analysis. The Winesaps, though high in total sugars, are low in sucrose, indicating a heavy juice rather lacking in rich- ness. The comparatively high acid content corresponds to the sprightly subacid character of the fruit. The high- est acid content was found in the fruit from Cloverland, where it is associated with a total lack of sucrose result- ing in a comparatively poor fruit. The apples from Cashmere and White Salmon were also devoid of sucrose in the juice, but the acid content was low and the flavor- ing principles well developed, as a result of which the quality was fairly good. The poorly developed Winesaps grown at Pullman were deficient in sucrose, acid, and flavors and were correspondingly poor in quality. The Yellow Bellflowers, though low in total sugars, were rather high in sucrose and also in acid. The bal- ance between these constituents is good and results in a moderately rich, pleasant, subacid flavor. SuMMARY The opportunity for the study of apple variation was unusually good, owing to the facilities afforded for the examination of fruit from various localities and different environments, and it has been possible to work out the fundamental principle upon which variation resulting from external factors depends and to apply it in the study of environmental adaptations. This principle, the Law of the Optimum, states that, for any given variety there is for each character a certain intensity of each essential factor of the environment at which, other conditions re- maining the same, that character reaches its highest de- velopment. In the application of this law to varietal adaptations, the essential point is the proper balance between char- acters and environmental factors, that is, all factors should be of such an intensity as to permit a good all- round development of the fruit. In the absence of such No. 574] VARIATION IN THE APPLE 633 a balance certain characters may fail to reach a proper degree of development while others develop to excess. The failure in quality and other respects of many of the best dessert varieties of apples when grown in Wash- ington is due to such a lack of balance. Practically all of them originated under a much different environment and were selected and came into prominence owing to their perfect balance of adaptation in localities having a set of external conditions similar to those under which they originated. The hope of northwestern apple culture in the future lies in the careful selection of varieties and the origination locally of varieties of high quality showing adaptation to the conditions of growth in the various sec- tions. In the meantime plantings should be made from those varieties of high quality which show the best adaptation. These are Esopus, Yellow Newtown, White Pearmain and Delicious for the irrigated valleys, and Wagener, Delicious and McIntosh for the higher valleys of northern and eastern Washington. Jonathan, Stay- man and Winesap show a poorer balance and should not be planted too recklessly. The climate of the Pacific coast resembles that of western Europe more than that of the eastern states, and further importations of Euro- pean varieties is desirable especially for testing west of the Cascades. The moisture relation is probably the most important factor in inducing variations, and is doubtless responsible for certain variations which have been ascribed to other causes which act indirectly by modifying the moisture supply. The elongation of the fruit following a cool period after blooming may result from a diminished cir- culation of the sap, giving rise to an insufficient supply to provide for the simultaneous development of the fleshy portion and elongation of the axis. Variation in the depth of the cavity and basin in certain varieties is prob- ably to be explained in a similar way. Color modifications depend to a great extent upon the light relation and somewhat upon development as influ- enced by temperature. The optimum intensity for the 634 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (VoL. XLVIII production of red pigment is quite narrow in most varie- ties and poor color may result from either deficiency or excess. Latitude and altitude affect the color only as they modify the factors upon which color depends, caus- ing them to approach or recede from the optimum. The influence of elements in the soil is not well understood. It is probable that soils containing sufficient iron. for the proper development of chlorophyll contain an abundance for the production of red pigment in apples. Aside from such differences as depend upon the hand- ling of the fruit, variations in keeping quality appear to follow the law of the optimum in the same manner as the other characters of the fruit. Conditions which favor the best all-round development result, as a rule, in good keep- ing quality. Apples grown under irrigation are said to keep poorly probably because of their unbalanced adapta- tion to the environment. Certain factors which favor de- velopment and maturity are present in excess, resulting in overgrown or overripe fruit. Varieties differ in specific gravity according to the extent of intercellular spaces in the flesh and the open- ness of the core. Overgrown specimens are low in speci- fic gravity. As a rule, those lots which kept best in any variety had the highest specifie gravity. Chemical composition is associated somewhat with quality. High suerose content results in richness of flavor. Fruit of high quality has the sugars and acids well balanced and the flavoring constituents well devel- oped. A heavy juice is usually associated with a high content of soluble solids. Fruit grown under irrigation is ordinarily rather low in soluble solids. There seems to be no constant relation between the amount of sunlight and the production of sugars, and flavors appear to de- velop best in a relatively cool climate. SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION VARIATION AND CORRELATION IN THE MEAN AGE AT MARRIAGE OF MEN AND WOMEN SOMEWHERE in sociological literature we have met with the statement that whereas the mean age at marriage of men differs from district to district because of social and economic conditions, the mean age at marriage of women varies but little because of these factors. In view of the high ‘‘assortative mating” coefficient! for age of bride and groom, this statement seemed so remarkable as to be open to question, Its validity can be very easily tested provided the mean age at marriage of men and women from a series of districts differing in economic and social conditions are available. If the mean age of women is independent of these conditions, or far less depend- ent upon them than that of men, one should find (i) that the variation of mean age of brides is lower than that of mean age of grooms, and (ii) that for a series of districts the coeffiicent of correlation between the mean age of brides and grooms is very low. The only suitable series of data that we have been able to find is that given by A. Dumont? for the average age in years and months at first marriage of the males and females of the 87 departments of France. Grouping his data in classes of five months’ range, we find, in terms of months :* 1 See Lutz, Science, N. S., Vol. 22, pp. 249-250, 1905. For a general re- view of the literature of assortative mating see Harris, Pop. Sci. Mo., Vol. 80, pp. 476-492, 1912. 2 Dumont, A., Rev. Ecole Anthrop. see Vol. dey 163, 1904. 3 The aese given by the ungrouped d For Men For Women ENE SS E A S $37.87 + 83 284.45 + 1.01 Standard deviation rssi yerr 11.49 + .59 14.00 + .72 Seo iy 492+ 25 oe the shortness of the series, the results are in as good agreement could be expected. For Men For Women PEM TT PET EAE E SSE E O La ewe 337.76 + .80 284.43 + 1.03 Standard oe ey Uy bats ule Ke 11.03 + .56 14.25 + .73 3.26 & 17 6.01 + 26 4 wena Sheppard’s correction for the second moment. 635 Mean Age of Women. 636 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII -260— 325 335 34 L> 355 365 Mean Age of Men. We note that the women marry on an average about four years and five months younger than the men. have been told, their mean age at marriage both absolutely, as measured by the standard deviation, and relatively, as measured by the coefficient of variation is more variable than that of men. The difference in standard deviations for the ungrouped material is 2.51 + .93 and for the grouped records 3.22 + .92. These are 2.71 and 3.49 times their probable error, and hence perhaps significant. For the coefficient of variation, the differences by the two methods are 1.52 + .31 and 1.74 + .31. These are 5.69 and 4.96 times their probable errors and their significance is even more probable than those for the standard deviations. The correlation coefficient from the grouped data by the prod- uct moment method, using the means and standard deviations given above, is Contrary to what we No.574] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 637 Tmt ==.781 + .028.5 Thus on a scale of —1 to +1 the interdependence of mean ages of men and women is very close indeed.* Expressing the same relationship in terms of regression by the well known formula ~ (F7—5% wm) 47H fa (7 rm) +r m, where the bars indicate population means and the sigmas popula- tion standard deviations of m = males and f =— females, f=—56.474 + 1.009 m. Thus we see that each month’s increase in average male age is followed by .a month’s increase in mean female age. The fit of the straight line to the empirical means as shown in the diagram is excellent—considering the small number of the district means from which the equation is deduced. Thus the available data show that the mean age at marriage of women instead of being less variable from district to district than that of men is actually more variable—both absolutely and rela- tively, In short, there is, as far as our data go, no evidence for the assertion that while the time of marriage of men is closely de- pendent upon the complex of social and economic conditions that of women is. practically independent of them. We have published this note in the hope that it may suggest to some one with the opportunities of obtaining really adequate data an investigation of the problem which has several rather important points of interest. J. ARTHUR HARRIS, Roxana H. VIVIAN COLD SPRING HARBOR 5 The difference method applied to the ungrouped material gives r= .763 + .030. The difference is of no significance. 6 Possibly, however, the relationship is in part spurious. The mean of males and females were taken on the basis of the same N, or approximately the same N, for the various districts. Data for investigating this question are not available. The point should be borne in mind by a subsequent worker. 638 - THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. XLVIII DUPLICATE GENES Some interesting questions are raised by a recent article by ‘Gregory: ‘‘On the Genetics of Tetraploid Plants in Primula sinensis.’’ Reciprocal crosses of two races of P. sinensis were made. One cross gave entirely normal results in F, as regards chromosome number and hereditary characters. The reciprocal cross gave an F, generation which was sterile with the parents and produced only a giant variety in F,. This proved to have the tetraploid chromosome number. Experiments indicated that the genetic factors had also all been doubled, a very significant parallelism. Gregory uses the nomenclature AAAA, ast AAaa, Aaaa, and aaaa to represent all the possible conditions as regards a pair of Mendelian factors. He states that heterozygotes of the form AAAa should give gametes AA and Aa, and should pro- duce, on selfing, the zygotes AAAA, 2A A Aa and AAaa, and that the last class selfed should produce recessives. On the chromo- some theory of heredity, this assumes that the four chromosomes concerned are equally likely to pair in synapsis in any of the possible ways, a very interesting phenomenon if the assumption proves correct. But it is conceivable that two independent synaptie pairs may be formed. It may be that only chromo- somes from the same original race pair in synapsis. It is true that ‘the first of the original crosses shows that the chromosomes of the two races can enter into normal mitosis and presumably into‘synapsis with each other. But the reciprocal cross indicates, perhaps, that in the environment of the cytoplasm of ‘this cross, they can not enter into synapsis. If this condition continues in later generations, we should represent the zygotes as AAA’A’, AAA’‘a’, AaA’a’, ete. This is the way in which duplicate genes have been represented previously as by Nilsson-Ehle, East and Shull. With this representation, heterozygotes of the form AAdA’a’ could never give rise to recessives after selfing for any number of generations. Which hypothesis is true in this case could easily be deter- mined by experiment. The published results are not sufficiently explicit on this point. If the original cross were of the type AA X a'a’, producing in F, Aa’, the F,, AAa’a’, would be a homozygote on the second hypothesis, and recessives should never 1 Proc. Roy. Soc., B 87, 1914. No. 574] NOTES AND LITERATURE 639 appear. On Gregory’s hypothesis recessives should appear in later generations. On the second hypothesis, homozygous races of the types AAa’a’ and aaA’A’ would be obtainable, in appear- ance like heterozygotes. These would breed true indefinitely when selfed, but should give recessives in F, after crossing, as in a case proved by Nilsson-Ehle. SEWALL WRIGHT BUSSEY INSTITUTION, 4 Forest HILLS, MASS., ¢ June 19, 1914 \* \ NOTES AND LITERATURE A STUDY OF DESERT VEGETATION! Between three and four years ago Dr. W. A. Cannon, of the Desert Botanical Laboratory at Tueson, Arizona, visited southern Algeria in order to become acquainted with the more obvious features of the plant physiological conditions of the desert, and to make detailed studies of the root habits of certain desert plants. From Algiers the journey proceeded nearly due south about three hundred miles to Ghardaia, thence east about» one hundred miles to Ouargla, and another hundred miles to Ee gourt, returning through Biskra, and Batna to the northerneoast Throughout this long and wearisome journey the vegetation was studied in connection with the geographical and climatice environ- ment and the results are brought together in a volume of some- what more than eighty pages of text and thirty-seven plates, one of which is an outline map of the region visited. Dr. Cannon speaks of the similarity of the flora of Algeria to that of southern Spain, France and Italy, where one is reminded of the vegetation of portions of California. Once in the desert on the way south low-growing shrubs on the plain become char- acteristic, including species of Tamarix, Zizyphus and Artemisi. Where water is available for irrigation, oases oceur with the luxuriant vegetation of date palms, apricots, figs, mulberries! peaches, pears, oranges, as well as artichokes, beans, carrots, melons, peas, potatoes, squashes, ete. Further south the plain 1 Botanical Features of the Algerian Sahara. By William Austin Cannon, Washington, D. ©. Published by the Carnegie Institution of Washington, — 640 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII is covered with small stones and pebbles and ‘‘not a tree, shrub, or herb appears to hide the bare ground. The mountains are naked rock, while the harsh outline of desert ranges and the distant low sand ridges give no evidence of plant life. But a closer examination of plain, dune and mountains reveals the presence either of living forms or of the dried remains of plants of a preceding moist season, in numbers and in kinds not at first suspected.’’ All of which might well describe the desert condi- tions in our own southwest. This similarity is emphasized by the resemblance of many of the plants to those found in our Arizona deserts. Thus the ‘‘quidad’’ (Acanthyllis tragacanthoides) ‘‘has a very close resemblance to small specimens of ‘ocotillo’ (Fou- quieria splendens) of the southwestern United States.’? And this resemblance extends to the structure of the spines and the return of the foliage after rains. It is interesting to note that the natives burn off its numerous spines, after which the stems “‘are eaten with avidity by camels,’’ reminding us of the similar treatment and use of some cactuses in Arizoria. Further to the south the vegetation is still more sparse and xerophytic, includ- ing Ephedra, Retama, Haloxrylon, and among grasses, Aristi pungens. Near Ouargla, the southern point reached, there are places where no vegetation is present, as on the dunes, and yet on the fixed sand nearby were found Euphorbia guyoniana, Retama retam and Genista sahare. Much attention was given to the root habits of the plants encountered, and in the general summary which follows the account of the journey comparisons are made with the root habits of Arizona plants. With this meager introduction we must refer the reader to the volume itself, which it is quite impossible to summarize in these pages. One thing impresses itself forcibly upon the reader, and that is that a desert is a hungry place in which the permanent vegetation maintains itself against plant-eating animals by a thorny or spiny protection. Yet Dr. Cannon points out that in this character of spininess the American desert plants excel those of the plants of the Sahara region. CHARLES E. BESSEY THE UNIVERSITY OF NEBRASKA VOL. XLIII, NO. 575 NOVEMBER, 1914 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST A MONTHLY JOURNAL Devoted to the Advancement of the Biological Sciences with Special Reference to the Factors of Evolution CONTENTS Page I. A Comparison of the Responses of Sessile and ob Plants and Animals. fessor VICTOR E. SHELFORD 641 II. An Apterous Drosophila and its Genetic Behavior. CHARLES W. METZ - 675 III. Shorter Articles oy amea Formulæ for the Results of Inbreeding : Professor H. S. NINGS. ort-cut in the Computation of Certain Probable Errors : PEER B. Frost. Galton and Discontinuity in Vari- ation : Dr. R. giona GATES. The esua of a ae — Calf : - 693 Professor A. M. REES THE SCIENCE PRESS LANCASTER, PA. GARRISON, N. Y. NEW YORK: SUB-STATION %4 The American Naturalist SS intended for publication and books, etc., intended for review should be sent perei Editor of THE DOE shale NATURALIST, Garrison- -on-Hucson ee articles contai summa w York. ies of research work bear on the problems of organic alaiak are sonectaity welcome, and wiii be tues go Lo hae in or on. rea reprints of ear are supplied to authors free of charge. e hu Further 1 reprints will be supplie ubs s and advertisements should be sent to the Tigercat ear. twenty-five pal additional. subscription price is four dollars Canadian postage The Foreign postage is fifty cents and The charge for single aer En is page. forty cents. The advertising rates are Four Dollars for a p THE SCIENCE PRESS Lancaster, Pa. Garrison, N. Y. NEW YORK: Sub-Station 84 Entered as second-class matter, April 2, 1908, at the Post Office at Lancaster, Pa., under the Act ot Congress of March 3, 1879. E ARCTIC, pe deity and GREENLAND DS’ SKINS, Well oie Low Prices Particulars of G. DINESEN, Bird Collector Husavik, North Iceland, Via Leidle, England WANTED TO PURCHASE aset of BIRDS OF AMERICA by J. J. Audubon, 7 or 8 volumes, please report, stating cash price, stat- ing condition, binding and dates of volumes. F. C. HARRIS, Box 2244 Boston, Massachusetts For Sale Entire An important collection of Indian Birds’ Eggs, containing approximately 3,300. specimens of about 620 species, and in- | cluding among other varieties the Green- ish Willow Warbler (Acanthopneuste viridanus), Red-browed Finch (Calla- canthis burtoni), Tibetan Sand-Grouse (Syrrhaptes tibetanus), Spotted Sand- Grouse (Pteroclurus senegallus), Ibis- bill (Ibidorhynchus struthersi), White- bellied Minivet (Pericrocotus erythro- pygius), Bearded Vulture (Gypaétus barbatus), numerous Cuckoos with their hosts, etc., ete. Particulars and full list may be had from W. F. H. ROSENBERG 57 Haverstock Hill London, N. W. Marine Biological Laboratory Woods Hole, Mass. INVESTIGATION Facilities for research in Zoology, Entire Year arg Stade Physiology and ae of members of the staff. for such a table is $50.00. Courses of laboratory instructi ures are offered in inarte- vigie to August 11, $50.00. Phil hical Aspects of Bi and Alli camara td SUPPLY . =. gens DEPARTMENT Open the Entire Year wi o State which n and sli cn tS jal GEO. M. GRAY, Curator, Woods Hole, Mass announcement will be sent on application ox The Laboratory, Woods Hole annual The Director, Mario Mass- THE AMERICAN NATURALIST Vout. XLVIII November, 1914 No. 575 A COMPARISON OF THE RESPONSES OF SESSILE AND MOTILE PLANTS AND ANIMALS PROFESSOR VICTOR E. SHELFORD, UNIVERSITY OF ILLINOIS E AAA ai'e's 5 ok Soda Chg 4 Hs CRE ee Ca oes Cia te eas 642 IL. Basis of Discussion R E E hehe T A E E N A 642 aE AE PR ually ENE EN P E EE TO CV a E R E RS 643 . ` Sessile yer Motile si rie ERC EWN EA ROMP PE MICN Se wes The er pee and it + Relation i in fe and Groups ...... (a) A and Plants made up of Single Individuals .... 644 (b) Colonial ~ Multiple Individualed Plants and Animals. 645 i; apas — éro df Individus 8 kins ei cs Ca eri 645 ii SE is ne Kgs ck be eee a cia eee e ew sa wes ee iii. Metabeli and Reproductive Relations of Individ- Ses Wak 6 Wine SR wt eo eee air pe bee Chae 6 647 (e) Response ‘ot atin E TAS a, PE e EE AR oa STORE KEME SC A E E 6 Str a a aao 5004 oS £5 050 01S oa Fa cece a (d) Response of a ile j nt A EE EN E A 651 COUR) DOMINO iiss cae oe i dev enrarir enti 651 ii, orenak _ ROR E PSUS ere non T S ete 653 (e) Behavior of Sessile-motile Organisms .............. Response and Taxonomy of Sessile Danaa E E 653 (f E TS Saeg Sessile aad Motile Organisms with reference ad Aii Cg Seren E S E E Lae EE oar TA ear ee 1. ews eee pi parmtla go crea, pear pA Ue Sine he erga 2. rakte AA of Sessile and g ws a x Asst Biota .... 655 3. Sessile Motile Organisms in Ecological Succession .......... IV. Influence of Res nse Phenomena poe Biological Theory and Con- 3 rove i. Teleclogical voce Sempre, eee ire oe ae eee cr Bs NEDA othon VOW o.oo cli ees hs ce eee seiesvseeses ees 3 ee ae ng eta of Response and the Germ Plasm 4. Pasas of the ‘Study ‘of Response on Present- -day Biological ASO a PE T A E E re O E eT ee 5. Aspects of the Untenability of the Germ Plasm Doctrine.... 662 “3 ees of Values in —— RAPD es ne ones aes cess n Ra a EEA TA N T ook S a a V. re bea RN ins eke S cake eeee vee ashes ehesees 672 642 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVII I. INTRODUCTION Durine the past few years the attention of biologists has turned more and more from those phenomena which were supposed to be comparatively fixed, to responses to stimuli. Physiologists have long been concerned with the mechanism of response; psychologists are interested in its modification. Geographers, climatologists and ecologists have recently turned their attention to re- sponses in natural environments and zoologists have become interested in response, particularly from the point of view of its specificity. In these quite independent investigations and compilations there has been little attempt at analysis with a view to determine legitimate ‘lines of comparison among the exceedingly diversified types of organisms which have been investigated, and some confusion has resulted. For example, since the more obvious responses of plants are structural, persons not familiar with comparable phenomena among animals have made erroneous comparisons of sessile plants and motile animals. This paper is written to present in as nearly uniform terms as practicable (a) analysis of kinds or aspects of response, (b) justifiable kinds of compari- son, and (c) the bearing of response phenomena on biological theory and controversy. It aims to show that the numerous kinds of response are reducible to a few simple types common to both plants and animals, and that the failure to consider all types has been responsible for confusion and various one sided theories. It further aims to show that study of response during the past few years has led to an unusual broadening of our conceptions. II. BASIS OF DISCUSSION As a basis for discussion we must first have a clear understanding of the character and definition of response. Secondly, we must determine what constitutes an indi- vidual in those plants and animals that are made up of repetitions of parts. Thirdly, we must note whether or not the organism is sessile or motile, capable of playing the part of either, or colonial pelagic. No.575] RESPONSES OF PLANTS AND ANIMALS 1. RESPONSES The word response is used in various slightly different senses. In general it refers to more complex and time- requiring phenomena than ‘‘reaction.’’ In geography the term has been used (Goode, ’04) to cover all changes in culture supposed to be produced by climate or other geographic conditions. It is also applied by geographers and geologists to changes in the physical characteristics of man (evolution) which Goode (’04) has stated are slower than the cultural responses. In general botanists have used the term to cover changes of plant structure and function induced by external conditions.. Cowles (711), however, uses the word ‘‘reaction’’ to cover these phenomena. Coulter (’09) used the term response as synonymous with adaptation in plants. Zoologists have used the term to apply to changes in animals due to exter- nal conditions, but with little agreement as to what is to be included. We will use it here to include reactions, changes in functions, structure, color, induced by external conditions either directly or indirectly, without regard to how simple or how complex the processes involved may be.! The length of time required to bring the changes about may arbitrarily be taken as not exceeding the time required to breed five to ten generations of the species concerned. All organisms respond to stimuli because each stimulus acts upon some internal process. Strictly speaking, the response is the change or changes in the physical or chemical processes of the organism (or the part or parts concerned) which results from the disturbance. Those things which we commonly see and term response are often the later and less important phases of the dis- turbance. The striking phases of responses of motile organisms are usually movements which follow closely upon stimulation. In sessile organisms the noticeable responses often appear only after a considerable period. In both sessile and motile organisms some responses are i 1 For good representative bibliography see Adams, °13, Ch. VIII and x ~ 644 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII not evident because they concern internal, chemical and physical processes which affect neither form nor move- ment. Changes in the enzymes secreted by digestive glands, which accompany changes in food (Jennings, 06, p. 347), are examples. While thus recognizing that re- sponses are concerned primarily with internal processes, we must of necessity refer chiefly to the external phases. 2. SESSILE AND MOTILE ORGANISMS Sessile organisms are those which are sedentary in habit, whether attached or possessing slight powers of locomotion. Motile organisms are those that habitually move about. Vagile or creeping forms as well as swim- ming, walking, flying, burrowing types are included. Most sessile animals are capable of moving their parts, while only a few sessile plants possess this capacity, and these only to a slight degree. There is no sharp distinction between sessile (seden- tary) and motile organisms. Every possible gradation exists between fixed non-motile types as trees on the one hand and the pelagic fishes on the other. It is the extremes which we will compare. 3. Tue [INDIVIDUAL AND Irs RELATIONS IN COLONIES AND GROUPS The following comparison of animals and plants is an attempt to distinguish potential or incomplete individuals in colonial organisms and compound organisms which, while not commonly recognized as colonial, are made up of incomplete individuals. (a) Animals and Plants made up of Single Individuals The vast majority of animals belong here. Most pro- tozoa, solitary sponges, solitary hydroids, sea anemones, worms not preparing for asexual division, echinoderms, mollusks, arthropods and vertebrates. Only single-celled plants, young seedlings and possibly a few adults of multi- cellular plants which possess but one growing point No.575] RESPONSES OF PLANTS AND ANIMALS 645 (exclusive of roots) belong in this group. Single indi- viduals as described here are the basis for determining what shall be called individuals in colonial and compound types. (b) Colonial or Multiple Individualed Plants and Animals A number of animals and the vast majority of the plants belong here. The group can be roughly divided into two types, (a) those having a chain or plate arrange- ment of incomplete individuals and (b) those having a branching or tree-like arrangement. The groups of in- complete individuals of type a occur among the Protozoa, worms undergoing asexual reproduction, many of the Bryozoa and some of the Tunicates; both sessile and pelagic (plankton) forms occur. On the plant side type a includes plate-like colonies of alge, filamentous alge, some thallose plants and probably some of the fungi, though the great multiplicity of forms makes the separa- tion of this group from the branching tree-like types, difficult. Type b includes some of the colonial Protozoa, the majority of the sponges, hydroids, corals and the branch- ing Bryozoa. The alge, fungi, mosses, ferns and flower- ing plants are all represented. The colonies are usually attached to the substratum (sessile). i. Numbers of Individuals——Among the animals the number of so-called zooids is the number of incomplete individuals. In the sponges there are as many zooids as there are excurrent openings (oscule) (Minchins, ’00, p. 91). Zooids usually possess a mouth opening and organs for securing food, though in some cases they may be specialized for reproduction, defence or locomotion as in some of the Celenterates. Among the colonial plants there are as many incomplete individuals as there are buds or growing points (vegetative regions). There are no regularly occurring organs in animals, strictly com- parable to leaves. However, any organs such as tentacles, gills, ete., which secure or absorb nutriment may be re- 646 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (VoL. XLVIII garded as analogous to leaves. Each potential bud with its leaf may be compared to a zooid. In comparing plants and animals, roots can perhaps be compared with the holdfast organs of hydroids. In both groups, roots and root-like organs are individuals of a very low order of individuahization and of a type not well represented among animals. The holdfast organs of animals are not important absorbers of food and water. ii. Stems and Other Connecting Organs (Conducting Tissues).—The most striking difference between the in- complete individualed or colonial plants and colonial ani- mals is the presence in the former of specialized stems and highly complex conducting tissues (Cowles, 711; Piitter, "11, pp. 861-66). The conduction of food materials from _ the root to other parts of the plant and from the leaves to the root is a functional necessity not paralleled even in those colonial animals showing the greatest division of labor. In animals stems are relatively undifferentiated and are often made up of living, relatively unspecialized zooids, as, for example, in many Bryozoa such as Crisis. The tendency to cauliflory in some plants and the ability of cambium to produce shoots and of the stems of most hydroids to produce individuals indicates that such a con- dition may be potentially present in all. In stalked Protozoa the stems are solid, while in most Coelenterates they are tubes, usually simple though sometimes complex, made up by mere elongation and branching of the stock. of the simple single forms such as the Hydra. The lumen is usually ciliated and makes possible a transfer of mate- rial which renders practicable such division of labor as occurs in this group (Piitter, 711). In the Bryozoa the different zooids have their body cavities joined in the simpler forms merely as a branching lumen of the main wall of the colony; in others by small openings the more specialized of which are sieve-like plates (Harmer, ’01, pp. 471 and 496; Delage and Herouard, ’97, Vol. 5, p. 62). The connection between the individuals of the tunicate colonies is often very complex, due to the fact that in the No.575] RESPONSES OF PLANTS AND ANIMALS 647 most complex types the stolon (stem) gives rise to new individuals and possesses all the layers of cells which take part in forming them. The connection between different individuals differs in different groups and is determined by the particular mode of asexual reproduc- tion. As the individuals are quite independent of one another in function, these connections do not have the Same significance as in plants. Even where there is a common blood cireulation, as for example in the Clavel- linide (Harmer, ’04, p. 71), there is no noteworthy divi- sion of labor. — iii. Metabolic and Reproductive Relations of Individ- uals.—The flat worms at certain times consist of chains of zooids at various stages of development and with various degrees of independence. Child (713) has found that these chains of zooids present a series of gradients in rate of metabolic reaction. The rate is highest at the anterior end of the whole chain and decreases toward the posterior end, not uniformly, however, for the rate is lower immediately in front of each head region than it is in the head region itself. A gradient is present in the axis of each zooid. The most anterior head dominates so long as the chain remains intact. In the corals certain zooids dominate (Wood-Jones, ’11) over the others. Some types have a single dominant zooid and some more, ` while in other cases all are equal. Among plants whose form is that of a chain or a plate the individuals are less closely bound together and domi- nant vegetative regions are probably less well developed. In the branching types, dominant vegetative regions occur (Cowles, ’’11, p. 747; Goebel, ’00, Vol. I, p. 206). In the conifers, for example, there is a leader, a dominant grow- ing region at the tip of the main stem just as in certain madrepore corals (Wood-Jones, p. 83). Other plants like the elm have several vegetative regions which dominate over others, as they do in the branching madrepores. Growth form or colony form varies according to cer- tain laws dependent, in part at least, upon the metabolic 648 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII relations of individuals. Thus Wood-Jones says of the corals— a colony may grow according to five different types of vegetative growth . it may grow as (1) a spherical mass, (2) an encrusting layer, (3) a free plate, (4) a branching tree-like growth, or (5) a mere amorphous lump. He further notes the division of all the corals into two groups of normal growth-forms; for all the zooids may take an equal share in the asexual reproduction or, again, some may be of greater importance than others, and the asexual reproductive functions may be lodged in a very few individuals only. Considering the first division (all zooids taking equal share, the principal types of bud- ding vary from each other in the actual site of origin of the daughter zooid from the parent, in the degree of final separation of the two zooids, and in the thickess of the intervening partition between the two zooids. The amount of rising above the general surface by each indi- vidual zooid is likewise subject to variation. Turning now to the corals that constitute the second class (some zooids of greater importance than others) which in the words of Wood-Jones have some of their units specialized as active agents of growth, it is at once seen that the possibilities of variation of normal vegetative habit are greatly increased. 1 the elaborate branching forms, plates and leaf-like growths belong to this class; and all are evolved by special peculiarities of the growing point. The zooids that constitute the grow- ing point may take various forms; they may be arranged as a cluster, as a creeping edge, or as many varieties of terminal shoots of branches. In the first instance, it is necessary to draw very sharp distinctions between two subdivisions of this group. In Group 1 come all those forms like Montipora, whose distal zooids are the newest formed mem- bers of the colony; and in Group 2 are included the PEAT whose distal zooid is the most ancient individual in the whole p. In dealing with Group 1 many forms have to be pate Hy for when the youngest are the active zooids their growth cluster may be variously disposed, and on its disposition the resulting vegetative form entirely depends. In Group 2, however, this state of things is entirely altered, for there one zooid, which is situated at the extremity of the stem, and which I No.575] RESPONSES OF PLANTS AND ANIMALS 649 shall call throughout the “ dominant apical zooid,” constitutes the grow- ing point; and this zooid is the parent of the entire colony. Various writers make comparable statements or show comparable principles among hydroids (Motz-Kossowska, 08) and Bryozoa (Davenport, ’91, et al.) and among plants (Goebel, 700). Of the colony form of the tunicates Herdman (’04, p. 82) says: The marked differences in the appearance of the colonies of compound Ascidians is largely due to the methods of budding; even in those of stolon type where the budding is practically the same in essential nature, the results may be different in superficial appearance, according as the buds are formed on a short stolon close to the parent body, or from the extremity of the post abdomen or from the long epicardiae tube which may extend for some inches from the ascidiozooid. Thus we conclude that the innate causes of different growth-forms (colony forms) of colonial organisms are (a) the mode of division of the zooids or vegetative regions, (b) the ratio of stem elongation to number of zooids or buds produced or uniformity or lack of uni- formity of stem elongation (Wood-Jones, p. 76) closely related to (c) the presence or absence, number, position and region of influence of the dominant growing regions or dominant zooids, and (d), in some cases, the grand period of growth and the length period of the internodes (Johnson, 711). The innate tendencies are thus reducible to a few principles applicable to both plants and animals. (c) Responses of Motile Organisms i. Movements.—In motile organisms the most striking responses are changes in position brought about by movements usually more or less-random, and which bring the organism into various conditions one of which usually relieves the disturbance. The organism resumes normal activity in conditions which brought the relief (Jennings, 06). These conditions are not necessarily advantageous, but are usually so when the stimuli are those encountered in nature (Mast, 711). Behavior of motile organisms is also modified by repetition of action even in animals as low in the animal series as the Protozoa (Holmes, 711). 650 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII Jennings (‘06) has quoted various botanical workers’ observations on motile plants the behavior of which prob- ably follows the general laws governing the behavior of motile animals. As a result of the quick behavior re- sponses of motile organisms, their distribution at any given time is a better index of the conditions at that time than the distribution of sessile organisms, because when the conditions at a given point become unfavorable the motile organisms usually move to another situation, while the sessile forms remain and perhaps die. ii. Structural Responses—Among motile animals, structural and color changes occurring as a response to environmental conditions (stimuli) are usually not of importance to the organism concerned. The color differ- ences induced in Lepidoptera by heat and cold (Stanfuss ; Fischer) and the structural differences in Crustacea such as were brought about in Cladocera by Woltereck, and other modifications brought forward recently, are usually of no known advantage or disadvantage to the animals concerned (Bateson, 713, Ch. IX and X). Such re- sponses in color and general form do not ordinarily take place in adults subjected to such conditions. The strik- ing structural responses of motile animals are often responses to the organism’s activity. The use and disuse phenomena of the Lamarckians, the increase in size and form of muscles, thickening of skin in man and mammals, are well-known examples of a type of responses which have influenced zoological speculation. Child (’04) con- trolled the form of Leptoplana by controlling activity. Holmes (’07) found that the movements of pieces of Loxophyllum have an important part in shaping the general outline of the bodies of the resulting forms. The general forms of motile animals are correlated with their activities but whether form or structure correlated with it appeared first in the course of evolution has been the subject of considerable fruitless speculation. No. 575] RESPONSES OF PLANTS AND ANIMALS 651 (d) Responses of Sessile Organisms i. Structural Responses——The striking phases of re- sponses among colonial sessile organisms are often changes in form and structure, or the relative position of the parts. The changes in structure or position of parts are not necessarily advantageous or useful, but are usually so when the stimuli are those commonly encoun- tered in nature (Cowles, ’11; Loeb, ’06, p. 124; Wood- Jones, 711; Ch. VIII). Indifferent and detrimental re- sponses are often given under experimental conditions and no doubt the absence of such variants among sessile ani- mals collected in a wild state is due in part to the failure of such organisms to survive. A few sessile colonial organisms such as cacti (Cowles, 711) show little or no plasticity. Among sessile animals, the observations of Wood- Jones form the belt examples of response. He found that the branching type of corals dominated in barrier pools, tall slender non-branching types in deep water, and massive boulder types on surf beaten shores. Thus he figures similar colonies of each of three genera which, while possessing certain peculiarities of their own, are in general agreement as to growth form just as sessile plants usually are; and this in part for comparable rea- sons. Thus various conifers occur as Krummholz in the high mountains, due to severe conditions (Cowles, 711, p. 732), wind, snow, and in part to the injury of terminal growth regions of the main stem which gives rise to lateral branches. The boulder-like corals with the zooid at the same level occurring on the surf-beaten shores of coral islands are due, in the case of Madrepora, for ex- ample, to repeated injury of the terminal dominant zooids. Conifers in protected situations often grow into tall slender trees comparable with the (deep) stil-water corals. The barrier pools afford conditions where the | terminal buds are less often injured than in the surf and the tree-like branching corals result from minor injuries to dominant zooids. 652 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII Wood-Jones finds further that still-water corals are less strongly calcified than those in rough water, the strains producing increased secretion analogous to in- creased tissue production as a result of mechanical strains in plants (Cowles, p. 669). Corals show different kinds of growth under different environments partic- ularly when injured. The new part may be different from the rest and adjusted to the environment thus making it appear as though two ‘‘species’’ occurred in the same colony. The mode of division of the zooid is also different under different conditions. Plants show similar variation with changes of conditions, particularly in the leaves which are divided in submerged portions of amphibious plants and entire in the emerging portions (Cowles, 711, p. 595). As has been noted, there is nothing in sessile animals that is more than roughly analogous to leaves. Leaves show marked structural differences on different parts of the same tree where the environmental conditions are different, as, for example, in the differences which occur between the upper and lower portions of a forest tree. While there are, no doubt, differences in similar details (histology) in the organs of display in different parts of the same colony of sessile animals, little or nothing has been done upon them. As a further indication of the prevalence of structural response in sessile organisms of the hydroids Hickson states that there is probably but one species of Millepora which occurs in a large number of growth forms. The commercial sponges (Moore, ’08) and common freshwater sponges and polyzoa show many different forms under different environmental conditions. The major differences in growth form induced by ex- ternal stimuli in colonial organisms result from modifica- tions of the rate and character of growth with respect to = the four innate tendencies toward various growth or colony forms discussed above, and which may be briefly enumerated as follows: (a) mode of division, (b) amount _ of stem elongation, (c) influence of dominant regions and No.575] RESPONSES OF PLANTS AND ANIMALS 653 (d) grand period of growth and the length of period of internodes. The principles are concerned with asexual reproduction and apply to motile organisms only exceptionally as for example in the case of colonial pelagic forms. The laws are applicable to both plants and animals. ii. Movements.—Movements of sessile animals are usually contractions or extensions of parts or of the entire body. Tentacles and comparable organs are capa- ble of movements for securing prey. Such organs often tend to wrap about objects which are in motion. Many Sessile animals are capable of opening and closing a mouth opening and of bending or twisting the entire body. Plants possess a comparable capacity only occasionally. (e) Behavior of Sessile Motile Organisms Most sessile animals are capable of some movement and react by contraction of parts. The reactions may be modified by repeated stimulation (Jennings, ’06) and usually by physical factors. Some animals, as Hydra, Stentor and many others are both sessile and vagile or free-swimming, and show different types of behavior when attached and when free. Jennings states that such protozoa have a more complex behavior than motile forms. This is due to their combining the types of behavior of sessile and motile animals, (f) Response and Taxonomy of Sessile Organisms Hickson (’98) has stated that there is but one species of Millepore and believes that sex organs will be found to be the best taxonomic characters. Wood-Jones states that there are far fewer species of corals than has formerly been supposed, and states further that growth form can not be used to distinguish species. Among fresh-water sponges and Bryozoa reproductive bodies (gemmules and statoblasts) have been found to possess Satisfactory taxonomic characters. This is a situation quite parallel with that in plants where reproductive 654 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vov. XLVIII organs are used as classification characters. The ideas of the reproductive organs of plants are now at the “fixity”? stage which on the animal side is paralleled by the idea of fixed tropisms and fixed instincts, of a few years since. Variability of tropisms is now well recog- nized and reproductive organs in plants are being found plastic, as those of animals will probably be found also. III. PARALLELISM BETWEEN SESSILE AND MOTILE ORGAN- ISMS WITH REFERENCE TO ECOLOGY From a summary of the considerations above it will be seen that for practical comparison the division of organisms into plants and animals may be abandoned and only reference to sessile and motile organisms made. We may now turn to a discussion of a few general principles making the division into sessile and motile organisms only. The behavior of motile organisms is plastic. There are innumerable cases of modification of reaction by variations of physical factors (Jennings, ’06; Loeb, ’06; Mast, ’11). If for purposes of discussion we put the usual ‘‘normal’’ reactions of motile animals over against ‘‘normal’’ structure of sessile animals, we note that the behavior response of the former parallels the structural response of the latter. 1. BREEDING Motile Organism Fixed. (Sessile) Organisms (a) The breeding activities take place within narrower limits ae -= other activities. Merri 790; Herrick, 02; Reighard, "08; Shelford, "11a, b, c, 12a, b. (b) The selection of breeding place and breeding activities, in- eluding first activities the young, are governed by the same _ general laws as other activities. (a) Breeding and other activi- ties within same limits, except that dispersal may take place over wide areas through detachability of seeds and other reproductive bod- les. (b) Less marked because a se- lection of abode by sessile organ- isms takes place through the be- havior of motile young stages or through wide dissemination of non- motile bodies by wind (ete.) with growth under favorable conditions and failure elsewhere. No.575] RESPONSES OF PLANTS AND ANIMALS 655 (c) The breeding activities are (c) The reproductive organs probably least modifiable and least and early embryonic stages are regulatory. less modifiable than the vegetative parts. (a, b,c) The maple tree, a sessile organism, is entirely stationary in its adult stages. The seeds are blown by the wind. One would not accomplish much in the study of ecology by studying the distribution of the seeds of the maple, or, on the other hand, by the study of the distribu- tion of adult birds, without some further discrimination. Sessile organisms are not difficult to associate with their proper environmental conditions in their adult stages. As we proceed in our study to forms which can move readily and rapidly, the difficulty of associating them with their definite environmental conditions in- creases. Sessile organisms have stages which are small and capable of easy dispersal, as in the case of the maple. Sessile marine animals and some sessile plants frequently have motile forms in young stages. In these motile stages they are governed by the same laws as other motile organisms. The conditions under which the motile stages develop into the sessile forms are crucial. Most fresh-water forms and some marine forms of sessile organisms are without the free-swimming stage, and they produce non-motile stages physiologically comparable to the seeds of higher plants. The winter bodies (statoblasts) of the Bryozoan (Pectinatella) com- mon near Chicago, and which is a strictly sessile organ- ism, are comparable to seeds and probably require ‘‘ripening’’ by cold, just as do many seeds and the repro- ductive bodies of some other species of the same group. Organisms which are highly motile in the adult stages are not motile in the egg and young stages. The eggs and young of birds, for example, do not move about, yet birds are the most motile of all animals. 2. COMPARISON OF THE SESSILE AND MOTILE ELEMENTS OF THE ‘Brora (a) The motile organisms of a (a) The sessile organisms of a given habitat usually react simi- given habitat (particularly plants) 656 larly to two or more stimuli not differing greatly in intensity from their optimum, i. e., the percent- age of positive or negative trials is essentially the same for standard intensities. There is also probably similarity in the rates of metabol- ism, ete. (b) The specificities of behavior such as the mode of moving the organs, e. g., of locomotion, and in some eases the combined results THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVII usually show coe functional rates, sue s similar rates of transpiration among sand dune plants. (b) The various structural de- vices which meet the conditions of the environment are ecologically equivalent. of different behavior reactions are similar and hence are ecologically equivalent. The size and efficiency of the organs are also involved. A testing, for example, of the rheotaxis of a large num- ber of brook-rapids animals has shown them to be strongly positive, and when active individuals only are considered the percentage of positive trials is very similar for the entire rapids community. Likewise they are in accord in their avoidance of sand bottom. Many of the animals have special means of attachment which may be brought into play with speed. As has already been pointed out elsewhere, ecological equivalence is illustrated here. The darters (fish) are strong swimmers and are able to live in rapids by virtue of their swimming powers and positive reaction, while snails meet the same general conditions through positive rheotaxis and the strong foot which enables them to hold to rocks. 3. SESSILE AND MOTILE ORGANISMS IN ECOLOGICAL SUCCESSION (a) Ecological succession is succession of ecological (physiological) types over a given area, due to changes of conditions which both cause migration of physiological types and transformation of such types as remain (Shel- ford, ’1la, ’11b, ’11d, ’12a, ’12b and citations). Changes of conditions are geographic, i. e., physiographic, climatic, No.575] RESPONSES OF PLANTS AND ANIMALS 657 etc., and biological (due to organisms). Sessile plants are the chief biological cause of succession on land and in fresh water, while sessile animals are the chief biolog- ical cause in the shallow portions of the sea, especially in coral reef regions (Wood-Jones, ’11). Sessile organisms are more important causes of succession than motile ones because they (a) build up the substratum with detritus and skeletons, (b) interfere with the movement of the surrounding medium, (c) cut off light from the sub- stratum where other organisms must reside and their own young secure foothold, and (d) they usually affect their own environments with excretory products more than do motile organisms. In general we recognize ecological succession of motile animals through the differ- ences of behavior which accompany changes in conditions. The differences are physiological; differences in behavior are the easiest index of the physiological condition. The character of nests, burrows, etc., are often good indi- cators also. IV. INFLUENCE OF RESPONSE PHENOMENA UPON BIOLOGICAL THEORY AND CONTROVERSY A glance at some aspects of biological speculation since before the publication of Darwin’s ‘‘Origin of Species’’ is essential to our understanding of the atti- tude of biologists until recently, toward responses. 1. TELEOLOGICAL View In the matter of animal behavior response, the earlier workers interpreted the reactions as intelligent and pur- poseful, ascribing human sensations, ete., to animals as low in the seale as protozoa. This teleological tendency was paralleled on the plant side by the idea of purposeful adaptive responses. Many common plants respond (structurally) readily to environmental conditions. As has been noted, the commonest of the surviving responses of the wild state are apparently advantageous. This led some botanists to a Lamarckian teleological conception of response, perhaps best represented by Kerner and 658 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vow.XLVIII ` Oliver’s work on the natural history of plants. Accord- ing to this view, responses are advantageous and for the purpose of preserving the plant. Thus response and adaptation become synonymous (Coulter, ’08), a usage quite inapplicable to animal structure. At the beginning of the recognition of the response phenomena of corals Wood-Jones takes essentially the view of adaptation which botanists have tried and rejected. Lamarck, who was for many years engaged in botanical work, must have noted many cases of advantageous structural response in plants. Later he undertook the study of invertebrates which show great plasticity, and was naturally much influenced in the development of his theory of transmutation of species by the response phe- nomena in the plastic organisms which he studied. Thus the responses of motile (as well as sessile) organisms which result from their own activities or the action of their environments formed an important feature of Lamarck’s (Packard, ’01; Cope, ’96) theory of transmu- tation of animal species. His theory is clearly in accord _ with the material he studied most. The nature of his contention and various well-known circumstances caused his ideas not to be accepted. 2, NATURAL SELECTION VIEW Characters used in classification of motile animals before and since the time of Darwin are quite frequently adaptation characters. Thus the large pectoral fins and absence of an air bladder are characteristics of an entire group of fishes, the darters. The divided eyes of the Gyrinide, which swim at the surface of the water, are so adjusted that one half looks downward into the water, and the other outward into the air. This character com- bined with the paddle-like hind legs would have served to distinguish the family. Again larve with a head and thorax modified to fit a circular burrow and with hooks on the dorsal surface of the fifth abdominal segment, which is supposed to be an adaptation to prevent the animals No. 575] RESPONSES OF PLANTS AND ANIMALS 659 from being drawn from their cylindrical burrows by their prey, could serve to distinguish the entire family of Cicindelide (tiger beetles). Such cases might be multi- plied indefinitely. Following Lamarck came Darwin, who, being more par- ticularly a zoologist, was probably (proportionately, at least) less familiar with structural response phenomena. He was apparently impressed with the ‘‘fixity’’ of the so-called adaptation characters in motile animals, and with the fact that they are often family, generic or specific characters. With the assumption that they originated in the environment in which they are now found, Darwin and his followers on the zoological side credited ‘‘natural selection’’ of structural characters with the origin of species. Though broader than Lamarck, this important feature of Darwin’s theory was quite clearly drawn from data on motile animals. After the acceptance of Darwin’s theory, biologists were for many years engaged in elabo- rating the ideas of phylogeny and natural selection by working out recapitulations and homologies and by point- ing out cases of adaptation. The investigation was largely confined to the highly individuated animals. The morphological method of this period, which indeed has still continued in use among a minority of zoologists and which finds a parallel in the recent morphological study of the sex organs of plants, belongs to descriptive rather than to analytical science. Since its conclusions are often based upon the arrangement of species or of stages in development into series chosen by the investigator, it is a method which often allows free play of subjective fancy. Thus unconsciously experimental study of modification by environment became more and more neglected, and the dominant type of investigation being such as to show only the usual course of events in development, the ideas of fixity grew more and more. Thus the fact that the external form, structure and color of animals are not easily modified without careful experimental methods, and that the structural responses of sessile animals were 660 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII so little known, resulted in structure in animals being fre- quently regarded as fixed and every resemblance and peculiarity being too often regarded as significant. The explanations of supposed adaptations among animals fell largely to the theory of natural selection which was strained by some (see, for example, in Romanes, ’92, p. 269) to explain origins in great detail, largely on the basis of the competition of species for food, ete. Explanations along this line were carried to a reductio ad absurdum as indicated by Livingston (’13) and have by no means dis- appeared from the scientific calendar. This tendency was less important on the plant side. More attention was given to speculation concerning adaptive response. From a consideration of the facts just presented, we note that the characters of the two leading early view points in evolution were no doubt influenced if not actually caused to crystallize into their peculiar form by the failure of workers to recognize the entire series of phenomena which we have presented above. Thus a review of the responses of sessile and motile organisms throws much light on the influences leading to the first conceptions and later modification of these two leading doctrines. Botan- ists for many years dwelt mainly on the response of sessile organisms and crystallized a Lamarckian conception of the origin of adaptations through the fixing of advantageous responses as hereditary characters. During the same period zoologists essentially ignored sessile and other multiple individualed animals and their great plasticity and crystallized the Darwinian idea into Weismannian germplasm doctrine based on highly specialized single individualed animals. 3. SUPPOSED Non-INHERITANCE OF RESPONSE AND THE ERM Puasm DOCTRINE The theory of the independence of the germ-plasm from the soma, and its continuity from generation to genera- tion, was brought strongly to the attention of zoologists in 1885 by Weismann. It was the natural outgrowth of the methods and theories of the preceding period and No.575] RESPONSES OF PLANTS AND ANIMALS 661 was largely based upon the non-inheritance of mutila- tions and the fact that the germ cells of a few organisms are, morphologically, early differentiated from the soma. Timing to its influence upon ideas concerning response, we note that from this viewpoint details of structure were not of fundamental importance unless traceable to the germ plasm. Still, structural details were more im- portant than response, because, with the exception of instincts, responses were believed to occur independently of the germ plasm and hence were of interest only on their own account. Thus the methods used in applying Darwin’s theory led to neglect of experimental study of response and culminated in the extreme views of Weis- mann. The germ-plasm theory or the ideas of heredity which are associated with it has dominated zoological thought almost if not quite down to the present day.” 4. THE INFLUENCE OF THE Stupy or RESPONSE ON Present-Day BrotocicaL THEORY One of the most striking developments of recent years has been the discovery that behavior responses are modi- fiable to a high degree. Small traces of reagents reverse 2 Unconsciously suggestions of the supernatural which come up in connec- tion with heredity and evolution have stimulated investigators to study and speculation, though they have often approached the question of heredity with an unscientific attitude. This is indicated by ‘such statements as ‘‘I could not, however, resist the temptation to endeavor to penera Fed mystery of this most marvelous and com a chapter of life’’ and ‘ momentous issues involved’’ and ‘‘no more crepes problem oe well be stated’’ bear out this sta HH ardeney which appears here and elsewhere in the discussion of A questions, ne to the writer to be associated with the discussion of pL which can not be referred to existing facts for solution. Few the ial caveat of scientific men acquired a working kaowledge of the methods of science before the age of twenty-five years, and the early habits of mind were formed in the atmosphere of the supernatural and dogmatic, which has characterized human thought for centuries. It is doubtful if the majority o stantly come back to our tests and principles. This may account for many of the contradictions regarding scientific safer te which one finds in the conversation of scientific men. When the methods of science have become the methods of society we may expect a ‘ile of scientific men far more effective than we ourselves can hope to be. $ 662 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII reactions. Intelligent behavior occurs in the lower Arthropods. Even Paramecium shortens the time re- quired to turn around in a tube, by repetition. Actions formerly regarded as instinctive now appear to be mere innate tendencies perfected by repetition. Thus the ideas of fixity have essentially disappeared from this field. The response of organisms to injuries and the general control of form in the lower groups has done much to break down the ideas of fixity developed by Weismann and embryological schools. Thus Child, the leading American worker in this line, is able to control size, form, number of eyes in the case of Planarians. Various writers have found modifications inherited after several generations of repeated stimulation (see Bateson, 13). The development of anti-bodies (immunity) has been shown to be a response occurring in connection with many normal processes. The discovery of responses of so many types has led to abandoning ideas of fixity even among students of embryology and genetics. Thus we note the recent decline of the doctrine of continuity and independence of the germ plasm and kindred doctrines and points of view, which constitute the central ideas of fixity. It will accordingly be profitable to consider some further facts which make the germ-plasm doctrine un- necessary. 5. ASPECTS OF THE UNTENABILITY OF THE GERM Puasm DOCTRINE The presence of primordial germ plasm is assumed even in sessile colonial organisms such as plants, cœlen- terates, and in flatworms, etc., where under certain con- ditions any small part of the body may give rise to a complete organism. Here the theory is not needed to explain the facts. Child (711) said: The theory of the continuity of the germ plasm as a system, inde- pendent of the soma, except as regards nutrition, has played an im- portant part in biological thought during the last two decades, but I am convineed that it has led in the wrong direction and that it is re- No.575] RESPONSES OF PLANTS AND ANIMALS 663 sponsible for many pseudo-problems of heredity and development, which on the basis of a different theory could never have oceupied the attention and wasted the energy of biologists. Briefly my position is, that the gonad primordium is, at least up to a certain stage of develop- ment, physiologically a part of the individuality as are other organs, and that its further history of differentiation into male and female gametes indicates that it becomes specified in a particular direction, at least partly in consequence of its correlative environment in the or- ganism. The independence of the germ plasm is not well sup- ported physiologically. Thus Wilson (712, p. 163) says of the effect of prolonged ingestion of alkaline salts by ‘mice: No obvious changes were evident in the liver, kidneys, lungs, spleen and intestines but in the testes some extraordinary alterations were found. These results are of especial interest because as the cells of the testes except the basal cells are regarded by many eytologists as out of “coordination with the somatie cells. As a result of these experiments it would seem that they are more susceptible to changes in reactivity than the surrounding plasma. Dungay (713) and authors cited have thrown compara- ble light on this question. The facts of embryology themselves are but a pseudo argument in its favor. The organisms in which continu- ity is supposedly demonstrable are highly individuated and their organs highly specialized and many different organs are early separated from the common mass of cells. The germ cells thus follow the general law of development in such animals. The germ plasm is prob- ably no more independent of other parts of the organism than is the liver or any other special tissue. ‘‘Germ plasm” and “‘ germinal continuity,” if such exist, may thus be merely incidental to the particular type of organi- zation of the specialized individuals in which they occur. It should further be noted that on the botanical side this doctrine of the independence and continuity of the germ plasm has received little attention and has n given little credence because ‘‘germ plasm’’ arises from different tissues and is neither set aside early from the soma nor is it in any other sense clearly continuous. 664 - THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII Furthermore, the plasticity of plant structures made the application of the doctrine of natural selection to sup- posed adaptations untenable, and this type of explanation has received little more attention with botanists than have Lamarckian speculations with zoologists. The adaptation characters of plants can not ordinarily be used as taxo- nomic criteria (Coulter, ’08). 6. Tur Measure or VALUES IN BIOLOGICAL SCIENCE One hears reference to pure science as something quite apart from applied science. It is indeed true that inves- tigators in pure science are to some degree prompted to push forward in research by interest in the problems for their own sakes. But the human mind does not work long isolated from practical affairs or the main channels of human interest, and it is doubtful if the pure-science investigator continues long in this way. Observations are soon connected up in some way, actual or possible, with some human interest, be it as remote as the improving of human stock in remotely future generations. Thus ‘‘ pure science” defined as investigation for investigation’s sake hardly exists so far as the pure-science workers are concerned, but may be best defined as an indirect method of attacking problems of general importance. It differs from applied science in that application to practical problems is not its aim, though the estimated value of theories and results in ‘‘pure’’ science are often greatly modified by applicability to practical questions. Certain problems and groups of facts in biology are sometimes referred to as fundamental. Some one has said that a fundamental problem is one the solution of which biologists have decided will give greatest progress. It is doubtless true that a few leaders reach such decisions with regard to particular questions, but the real causes of their general acceptance as fundamental are social and imitative. Thus when one investigator or a small group of investigators arrives at such a decision many others usually become active along the same lines largely because it is a popular topic. Thus under the influence No. 575] RESPONSES OF PLANTS AND ANIMALS 665 of a group of investigators among whom Weismann was a conspicuous leader, problems of the germ cells, the ege’s early development, and heredity, became ‘‘funda- mental problems.’’ They evidently argued that since all comes from the egg and germ cell, all must be discover- able in the egg. If germ plasm were as independent from soma, as completely insulated from environment as con- tinuous from generation to generation as has been assumed, the study of germ plasm would be the only way to the solution of the problems of heredity and evolution. This follows no matter whether the chromosomes or almost the entire egg are credited with carrying heredi- tary qualities; only the postulation of continuity and independence from soma and insulation from environ- ment are necessary. If the independence of germ plasm from soma be accepted even in a weakened and modified form it follows that studies of somatic characters can at most be of secondary importance from the point of view of heredity and evolution. Thus in some quarters the value of various lines of zoological work has been esti- mated largely, unconsciously, no doubt, in proportion to the nearness or remoteness of their relation to the “germ plasm’’ question. Thus it is true that in biology as in all other fields values are measured consciously or unconsciously by criteria. In recent years another better criterion of value has made its appearance among zoologists. The germ plasm criterion already discussed was primarily morpho- logical; the second is physiological, borrowed no doubt from physiologists. It measures values on the basis of the analysis. of the organisms into terms of physics and chemistry or is concerned with a mechanistic conception of life in all its manifestations. From this viewpoint the study of each and every part of the organism is important because the discovery of laws governing one part is usually or at least often of general importance. Investi- gations from this viewpoint have shown that the germ plasm criterion is clearly illogical in its application to the study of somatic characters because it is based upon the 666 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII tacit assumption that the soma is governed by different laws from the living matter which makes up the germ plasm from which it arose. In other words it is assumed that the germ plasm is so different from the soma that the discovery of laws governing the soma is a type of investigation of relatively little significance. Some criterion of values is of course necessary in sci- ence as well as elsewhere, and for the sake of argument we would be willing to accept the second when broadly stated and the first when broadened and modified so as to accord with the second as appears to be the case among certain students of genetics. In other words, problems of the germ cells, the egg, and heredity, are of much importance when the germ cells themselves are regarded as dynamic and in their relations to the dynamics of the organism as a whole. Granting that these are true and tenable criteria of values in present-day biological science, what. is to be the method of application? Should biology demand that results be of direct application to these ‘‘central’’ prob- lems? One has but to look at the history of almost any branch of science to find that great, if not the greatest, advances have come through following up results at points where relations to the central problems of the period were quite unsuspected, or by the transference of methods, principles and results from one field to an- other where relations between the two were not suspected. Take, for example, immunity and immunization, the his- tory of which is ably sketched by Adami (’08, pp. 451- 528). It has been known for ages that one attack of many infectious diseases yields more or less complete immunity from subsequent attacks. Thus for centuries in India and the East individuals, chiefly children, have been pur- posely inoculated with matter or by contact. The prac- tise grew out of experience showing that diseases thus — communicated to healthy individuals from weaker ones are less severe. In 1796 the results of Jenner on vaccina- tion with cowpox were published. This may have influ- No.575] RESPONSES OF PLANTS AND ANIMALS 667 enced Pasteur, who over eighty years later laid the foundation for the modern epoch of development, by combating a plague of diarrhea in poultry (1880). During the twenty years following, various investigators added noteworthy contributions, and about 1900 Ehrlich and Morgenroth evolved the ‘‘side-chain theory’’ by which a large number of possible conditions can be pre- dicted and all the observed facts of immunity explained. While not expressed in strictly chemical terms, the theory and the experiments which support it are very important both practically and theoretically. In recent years the knowledge of immunity and comparable phenomena have been greatly extended. Various workers (Pfeffer, Vol. II, p. 262) have shown similar phenomena in the increased resistance of plants to poisons, thus making the responses of plants and animals still more generally comparable. Most recently workers on problems such as fertilization (Lillie, 713), standing in close relation to the older germ- plasm doctrine, have discovered facts belonging to this field and made use of Ehrlich’s theory to explain the ob- servations. This development has helped to confirm the conclusion of some investigators that immunity phe- nomena represent important features of the chemical mechanism of life. Adami has remarked, That a plague of diarrhea in a poultry yard, studied by a professor of chemistry, should be the seed from which has grown the vast de- velopment of later years is a strange fact, but a fact nevertheless. What was the attitude of pure science so called, of germ-plasm doctrinairies, and biologists generally during the long period which elapsed before they could make use of his results? Clearly it was one of indifference, if not disgust, toward the subject. The probable result of such attitudes on the progress of the investigation of immunity phenomena, had it not been for their immense practical significance, is clear. They could not have received their proper share of attention. Thus in the pursuit of the analysis of the chemical mechanism of life men who sought it directly have failed in this one impor- 668 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII tant step, and the chief contribution has come from very remote indirect methods. Generally speaking the inves- tigators who choose a direct method of attack often put themselves somewhat in the position of the chemist who would make chemical analysis of living matter when his first step defeats its own purpose by killing the substance to be analyzed. The failure of exclusively direct methods is often evident. Still the ability to obtain results by the method of direct attack, combined with a far too rare ability to tie with them indirectly obtained data, some- times gives noteworthy contributions. It accordingly remains to be seriously considered whether or not biology can afford to apply criteria to the measure of the values of investigation. Their application is of course largely unconscious, but the effects are not thereby modified. Noteworthy results of their applica- tion are (a) concentration of work in certain lines indi- cated by a given criterion, and (b) an actual abandoning to a large degree of remote and indirect methods of attacking the problems which the criterion involves. This means the partial abandoning of the methods for which pure science stands. Criteria can be safely used only in a very broad gen- eral way, and in application more often to past progress than to current investigation. They are perhaps most valuable as a guide to individual investigators working on problems remote from these more or less central ‘‘pure science’’ questions. That some guide should be in the hands of such workers is beyond question. In the hands of those attacking the problems directly they often appear detrimental because they soon take on an extreme form and become regarded as fundamental. At this stage they are usually in need of extensive revision. If the investigator is contributing observations and details only, he is doing a great service, for, such information is needed everywhere. If he is able to combine his own and others results, he almost invariably draws data from all sources, direct and indirect, far and near. Granted the No.575] RESPONSES OF PLANTS AND ANIMALS 669 ability to synthesize, the opportunity to use the ability sometimes comes to those who attack the so-called cen- tral problems directly. It comes equally often (we believe more often) to those who have led up to the central prob- lem from some remote viewpoint, frequently condemned by the followers of direct method of attack. Granting the importance of synthesis, if the biologist seeks the solution of such a problem as the germ-plasm problem, he should encourage workers to start at points as remote from the subject as possible, that they may approach it with new light and from new angles. In judging the work of another, its value should be determined more by the (a) strictness of scientific method used, (b) the thoroughness and completeness of the in- vestigation, and (c) (and perhaps most important of all) evidence of ability to synthesize and combine other re- sults with his own with a view to broader generalization. It must, however, also be recognized that there are many biological problems of much human importance, which must be solved quite independently of the ideal central problems of pure science. 6. Summary AND CONCLUSIONS From the data presented above, we note that the doc- trine of purposeful, advantageous response (including anthropomorphic ideas) arose from the uncritical non- experimental study of the responses (structural) of ses- sile and (behavior) motile animals. The idea of the all- sufficiency of natural selection is largely the outcome of observational study of apparently fixed and yet appar- ently adaptive characters of motile highly individuated animals. The doctrine of the continuity of the germ plasm is likewise the outgrowth of the study of highly individuated animals in which the various organs are early differentiated in the dividing egg. No one of the doctrines is wholly tenable; no one is more than a partial truth. Each appears to have arisen from a recognition of certain more or less unconsciously selected and un- critically interpreted phenomena by each of several men 670 © THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [VoL. XLVIII who secured different facts and attempted explanations. In a few animals the ‘‘germ plasm’’ may be morpho- logically early differentiated and reasonably continuous, though governed by the same laws as other tissues. In: others, any part of the general tissues may give rise to a complete organism. The behavior of some organisms is intelligent and purposeful, while that of others is largely mechanical. Some structural responses of sessile organ- isms are advantageous, some indifferent and some harm- ful. Some of the more fixed structures of the highly indi- viduated animals are advantageous, some indifferent, and some disadvantageous (Metcalf, 713). No other type of general statement appears to be tenable, yet each extreme of each proposition has at some time or other been the subject of some all-inclusive doctrine. Such are the limitations of an individual’s knowledge and the psychic limitations of our race and generation. In considering the psychology of religion, Ames (’10, p. 594) points out similar well-recognizable tendencies in that field of human activity and quotes Cooley on social © development as follows: Much energy has been wasted or nearly wasted, in the exclusive and intolerant advocacy of special schemes—single tax, prohibition, state socialism and the like, each of which was imagined by its adherents to be the key of millennial conditions. Every year makes converts to the truth that no isolated scheme can be a good scheme, and that real prog- ress must be advanced all along the line. Advance all along the line is what biological science must achieve. This I believe means the encouraging of all lines of indirect attack, whether they at first throw light on the ideal central question of pure science or important practical problems or not. It means the exer- cising of extreme caution in the application of criteria of values to scientific results. Such measures tend not only to stifle the best initiative in good investigators, but also tend to check the building up of fruitful hypotheses. The latter danger is greatest in connection with the = mechanistic criterion referred to above: As has already _ been stated, criteria of values ean be safely applied only No.575] RESPONSES OF PLANTS AND ANIMALS 671 as broad general guides, and investigation should be measured on the basis of its thoroughness, the originality shown, ete. In science special schemes of course do a exist recog- nized as such, but intolerant application of criteria of values results in essentially the same condition. One often hears the statement made by so-called scientific men, that this or that line of investigation has been pur- sued for several years, but has failed to yield important advances or generalizations, but they add, we will be very glad to recognize it as soon as its value is proven. This seems to us to be a distinctly unscientific attitude, and but a polite modern statement of a spirit which in former generations often sent men to the stake or dungeon. This is true because to these oe objectors its value is rarely or never proven. It is ‘‘schemes’’ (preconceived theories) thus presented that have in the recent past stifled the study of responses by discouraging efforts in that direction and thus contributed materially toward making zoology the unorganized science which it is to-day. We must recognize that the various aspects of zoology pure and applied have never been well corre- lated, less so we believe than in any other branch of natural science, clearly less than in botany. In general. animal physiology has been isolated in medical schools and genetics, faunistics and morphology have not been properly influenced by it, while morphologists for many years held themselves aloof from other workers. In a discussion dealing mainly with the doctrine of natural selection in the origination of adaptations, Mathews (713) has sounded the keynote of a growing attitude toward all response questions. Out of the infi- nite different combinations which may enter into the proteid molecule and the varying rates at which metabolic action may go forward, innumerable types of irritability and correlated structure have been and still are arising under the influence of environment external and internal. Of these some are incompatible with life, others indiffer- 672 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII ent, and others advantageous. Upon these physiological characters natural selection has operated to eliminate, and with time has perhaps rendered of less frequent occurrence, those characters that are incompatible with their conditions of existence. External form, color orna- mentation, ete., while no doubt often of importance them- selves are more often the advantageous or indifferent correlatives of physiological or irritability types which are compatible with their conditions of existence. The study of irritability and response may be pursued in many ways—by experiment, by observation in nature alone or combined with experiment. The mapping of stimulating conditions in nature, of the distribution of types of irritability and response, which is one function of field ecology and modern geography, can hardly fail to contribute materially to the advance of knowledge in -many lines, including that of the physico-chemical mechanism of life. The student of experimental ecology has an infinite field of problems and methods thrown open to him by the organization of such information relative to responses. Still in our attempt to make ad- vances along the line of the study of responses, we must not forget that it is but one of several lines of advance, all of which must sooner or later be correlated with a view to broader generalization. HULL ZOOLOGICAL LABORATORY, UNIVERSITY OF CHICAGO April 1, 1914 LITERATURE CITED Adami, J. G. (’08). Principles of Pathology; Vol. I, General Pathology. Philadelphia. i Adams, Chas. C. (’13). Guide to the Study of Animal Ecology. Chaps. II, IX. New York. Ames, E. S. (710). The Se of Religious ees, New York. Bateson, W. (°13). Problems of Geneties. New Hav Brooks, W. K. (’93). Salpa in its Relation to its Nvotitibi of Life. Johns Hopkins Univ. Studies. Bicl. Lab., Vol. 5, pp. 129-211. Child, C. M. (’04). Studies on Regulation, II. Jour. Expt. Zool., Vol. II, pp. 95-1 (711). A Study of Senescence and Rejuvenescence Based on Experi- ments with Planaria dorotocephala. Roux. Arch., XXXI, pp. 537-616 No.575] RESPONSES OF PLANTS AND ANIMALS 673 (713). — Dynamic Factors in Experimental Reproduction. x. Arch., Bd. XXXV, pp. 598-641. Coulter, J. oy (*00). The Theory of Natural Selection from the Stand- point of Botany. Fifty Years of Darwinism, pp 75. Cowles, H. -z (711). A Text-book of Botany. Vol. II, Ecology. New ork. Cope, E. D. eae Primary Factors of Organic Evolution, Chicago. Davenport, C. B. (’91). Cristatella: The Origin and Development of the SpE in the Colony. Bull. Mus. Comp. Zool, Vol. 20, -152. PP. Delage, Y., and Heron, E. (’96). Les Vermudiens, Zoologie Concrete BY: Dungay, N. S. (13). Pe Study of the Effect of Injury on Fertilizing Powers of Sperm. Biol. Bull., Vol. XXV, 213-260, Forbes, S. A. (’80). Some Interactions of Organisms. Bull. I. St. Lab. Nat. Hist., III, pp. 3-1 Goode, J. P. (’04). Human Responses to the Physical Environment. Jour, of Geography, III, pp. 333-343, Goebel, x (?°00). Organography of Plants. Part I, English ed., Oxford. Harmer, S. F. (’01). Polyzoa. Cambridge Natural History, Vol. TI, pp. 465-534. Herdman, W. A. (’04). Ascidians and Amphioxus, l. c., Vol. VII, pp. 35-138. Herrick, F. H. (’05). The Home Life of Wild Birds. New York. Hidtson, S. J. (’98). On the Species of the Genus Millepora. Proc. Zool. Soc. London, 1898, pp. 241-257. Holmes, S. J. (’07). The Behavior of Loxcphyllum and its Relation to Regeneration. Jour. Expt. Zool., Vol. IV, pp. 399-418. (711). Evolution of Pol Intelligence. New York. Henderson, L. J. (’13). The Fitness of the Environment. Am. NAT., eae ia 105-115. Jennings S. (706). Behavior of the Lower Organisms. New York. Jo dhnson, My E. irons A Qualitative Study of the Development of e Salpa Chain in Salpa fusiformis runcinata. Univ. of Cal. ac in Zool., Vol. 8, No. 7, pp. 145-176. Johnstone, J. (’08). Conditions of Life in the Sea. Cambridg Klebs, Œ. (’03). Willkiirliche Entwicklungsiinderungen bei Pflanzen. Jena. Lillie, Frank R. (’13). The Mechanism of Fertilization. Science, N. S., Vol. XX , pp. 524-528. Livingstone, B. E. (13). PEE in the Living and Non-living. Am. Nar., XLVII, pp. 7 gl J. (706). ‘Dynamics of Pia Matter. New Yor ugal, D. T. (’11). Organic Response. AM. NAT., Tok XLV, pp. 5-40. ‘ied S. O. (’11). Light and the Behavior of Organisms. New York. Mathews, A. P. (’13). Adaptation from the Point of View of the Physi- ologist. Am. Nart., XLVII, pp. 90-105. McGee, W J (’96). The Relations of Institutions to Environment. Smith. Rep., 1895, pp. 701-711. Merriam, C. H. (90). Results of a Biological Survey of the San Fran- 674 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII cisco Mountain re ate and the Desert of the Little Colorado. North American Fauna 3. Metcalf, M. M. (713). kantita through Natural Selection and Ortho- genesis. AM. NAT., Vol — Minchin, E. a tts Sponge. Takata: s Treatise on Foalog’: Part II. Moore, H. F. rey “The Commercial Sponges and Sponge Fisheries. Bull. S. F. C., 1908, pp. 403-511 Motz-Kossowska, 8. (708). Quelques asiaa à propos de Plumularia lichtensterni Mark eim et les via affines. Arch. Zool. Exp. et Gen. Notes et Revue, 4, T. IX, pp. 55-59. Packard, A. (’01). Lamarck the AFE of Evolution. New York. Piitter,-A, (711). on ae Physiologie. Jena. Reighard, Jacob (’1 ethods of Studying the Habits of Fishes with an ae ae the Breeding Habits of the Horned Dace. Bull. Bur. of Fish., XXVIII, 1908, pp. 1111-1136. Ritter, W. E. (711). The onkaan between Materialism and Vitalism. Can it be ended? Science, N. S., Vol. XXXIII, No. 847, pp. 437—441. Romanes, G. J. (’92-’97). Darwin and After Darwin. 3 vols. London. Shelford, V. E. (’lla). Ecological Succession, I. Stream Fishes and the Method of Physiographic Analysis. Biol. Bull., Vol. XXI, 9-35. PP. (11b), ee Succession, II. Pond Fishes. Biol. Bull., Vol. I, pp. 127-151, (’1l1c). GEE SG eS Animal Geography. Jour. of Morph. (Whit- an Vol.), Vol. XXII, pp. 551-618. (711d). Taigaa Succession, IIT. A Reconnaissance of its Causes in Ponds with Particular Reference to Fish. Biol. Bull., Vol. XXII, pp. 1-38. (’12a). Ecological Succession, IV. Vegetation and the Control of Land Communities. Biol. Bull., Vol. XXIII, pp. 59-99. (7126). Ecological Bora. V. Aspects of Physiological Classifi- cation. Biol. Bull., Vol. XXIII, pp. 331-370. Strassberger, E., Schenk, Noll F., and Schimper, A. W. F. (’03). A Text- oak of Botany. Translation by Porter and Lang. London. Stockberger, W, W. (°13). A Literary Note on the Law of Germinal Con- tinuity. AM. Nat., XLVII, pp. 123-128. Washburn, M. F. (09). The prng ESR Chap. I. Yor Weismann, A. (’91). 92, 2v. Essays upon Heredity and Faa Biolog- ical Problems. ATE Translation. E. B. Poulton & A. E. Shi pley. Oxford. (°93). The Germ Plasm a Theory of Heredity. Translation by N. W. Parker & H. Rönnfeldt. Wilson, F. P. (712). Cell Stimulation by Means of Prolonged Ingestion of Alkaline Salts. Biochemical Journal, Vol. IV, p. 162. Wood, Jones He 4D: On Growth Forms and spe apee Specter of Corals. ool. Soc. London, i pp. 518-556 (733), nie e Atolls. Londo: AN APTEROUS DROSOPHILA AND ITS GENETIC BEHAVIOR CHARLES W. METZ DEPARTMENT OF ZOOLOGY, CoLUMBIA UNIVERSITY Among the various mutants of the fruit-fly, Drosophila ampelophila, which have arisen from cultures in this laboratory, is one entirely destitute of wings, and hence called apterous.! The study of the heredity of this form has been difficult because of its almost complete (appar- ent) sterility. In order, therefore, to determine to which of the three groups of linked characters of Drosophila it belonged I was obliged, in most crosses, to make use of heterozygous flies that carried the factor for apterous. As this process is unique in certain regards, it will be described i in some detail. METHODS or STUDY At first it was thought that the apterous mutant was completely sterile, since none of the first flies, as they appeared occasionally in certain cultures, could be crossed even with normal individuals. At last, however, offspring were obtained from an apterous female by a wild male, and a permanent line started. But this line could not be perpetuated by means of apterous individuals, for these were unable to breed.? It had, therefore, to be kept up by means of heterozygous, winged flies. The method was as follows: The original cross of winged by apterous gave in F, approximately 3 winged to 1 apterous. Of the winged class approximately two thirds were heterozygous for apterous, and when mated together gave the same 1 This apterous fly is quite distinct from that called wingless in earlier papers by Morgan, and now known as vestigial. 2 Only twice, aside from the original mating, were apterous individuals successfully crossed, and then only to winged specimens, never to their own kind. These two cases are given in experiments II and ITI. 675 676 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII 3:lratio. Selecting again from the winged flies, the proc- ess could be repeated indefinitely. The only difficulty lay in the fact that no visible character differentiated the heterozygous from homozygous winged flies, and conse- quently all matings had to be made in pairs taken at random, with the result that about 56 per cent. of the cultures were rendered worthless. In actual practise large numbers were mated in pairs, and then all discarded save those producing apterous.* This was the method used in keeping up stock. To obtain the necessary combination of apterous with other mutant factors, winged offspring from apterous- throwing parents were mated in pairs to flies of the desired stock. One third of the normals from apterous stock were pure for the normal allelomorph of apterous and rendered worthless all matings in which they were in- volved; but the other two thirds were heterozygous for apterous, and when crossed with the desired stock gave in F, some apterous offspring. If the F, flies were bred en masse, approximately 15 winged to 1 apterous were ob- tained, but if bred in pairs, certain pairs (those in which both members were heterozygous for apterous) gave 3 winged to 1 apterous. The latter method was the one actually used in most cases. In this manner the same end result was attained as would have been secured by using apterous individuals in crosses with other stocks, the only difference being in the amount of labor involved in making up a larger number of cultures. Both kinds of crosses were, in fact, used, as will be seen below. The use of symbols in this paper follows the system recently adopted by Morgan and other students of Droso- phila (Morgan, 1913, a and b). That is, for any pair of allelomorphic characters a capital letter is used to indicate the dominant, and a small letter the recessive factor—the symbol being taken from the name of the mutant. Since the apterous character is recessive, the symbols for the 3 In the fourth experiment a character (black) was introduced which dif- ferentiated homozygous from heterozygous and thus made it possible to pick out the heterozygous individuals. No. 575] AN APTEROUS DROSOPHILA 677 apterous fly become arap, and those for the winged fly v-Ay. In other words, Ap is a factor in the wild fly necessary for wing production, while a is its modified homologue responsible for lack of wings in the mutant. The apparent contradiction in using Ap, not for the factor responsible for apterous, but for its normal allelomorph, may be confusing at first sight, but a little familiarity with the system obviates this difficulty. EXPERIMENTS Experiment I.—Long-winged, red-eyed & by apterous, white-eyed ? (from miniature wing stock). F, All winged. Long-winged, red-eyed females. Miniature winged, white-eyed males. F, Winged and apterous as follows: Long-winged, red-eyed males and females. Long-winged, white-eyed males and females. Miniature-winged, red-eyed males and females. Miniature-winged, white-eyed males and females. Winged Ast Apterous, red-eyed males and females. i as Apterous, white-eyed males and females. This experiment shows the inheritance of the apterous character to be Mendelian, giving in F, all winged, and in F, approximately 3 winged to 1 apterous. Table I con- tains a summary of the offspring from 21 pairs of the F, and F, individuals, giving a total of 1,405 winged to 450 apterous,—a ratio of 3.12 to 1. The absence of apterous flies in F, indicates at once that the apterous character is not sex-linked. The pres- ence of miniature-winged flies in F, and F, indicates that the apterous factor is independent of the miniature-wing factor, which latter must have been carried by the apterous female (coming from miniature wing stock), and trans- mitted to her offspring unaffected by the apterous factor. 678 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII Analysis of the cross: Ap, factor necessary for wing production. ap, its alle- lomorph, in the apterous fly. M, factor necessary for the production of long wings (sex-linked). , allelomorph of M responsible for miniature wings. factor necessary for the production of eye color (sex-linked). w, allelomorph of W responsible for white eyes. 38 P, Long, red male A,MW X—Ap, Apterous, white female a»mwX—apmwX. F, Long red females AMW X-amwX, Miniature white males AÁprapmwX. F., leaving out of account the sex-linked factors and considering only winged vs. apterous : Gametes of F, Ar, ap Ap, Ap. F. Ag-Co. Winged 4 Ap—Ap>. pp. Apterous dp—dp. Experiment II.—Long vermilion Ẹ by apterous, white 3.* This cross is practically the reciprocal of Exp. I, except - that vermilion replaces red eye color in the winged parent. Like Experiment I it involves two pairs of sex-linked char- acters, aside from the apterous character. The results are essentially like those of Experiment I and may be passed over briefly. P, Long, vermilion? A»MWX-—A,MWX, Apterous, white J apmwxX—dp.* 4 The white-eyed, apterous ¢ in this cross is white-vermilion, i. e., the double recessive, and therefore when crossed with vermilion it gives vermilion instead of red in F,. No. 575] AN APTEROUS DROSOPHILA 679 F, Long, vermilion? A»MWX-amwX, Long, vermilion ¢ A»MWX-«. F, Long vermilion Ẹ and g Long, white g : Miniature, vermilion ¢ Winged. Miniature, white ¢ bo Apterous, vermilion 2 and ¢ Apterous, white | Apterous. TABLE I OFFSPRING FROM PAIRS HETEROZYGOUS FOR APTEROUS IN EXPERIMENT I. PARENTS TAKEN FROM F,, F, and F, Mating No. | Winged Apterous Ratio 41 | 122 32 38:1 53 | 4 14 2.9 :1 56 | 29 12 25:1 64 | 46 30 15:1 65 | 29 16 1.8 :1 80 85 28 3.04: 1 83 71 20 < i 84 13 3 43:1 85 | 40 9 4k: i 111 | 183 64 98:1 112 20 10 S34 117 42 24:1 118 | 28 10 28:1 130 32 7 45:1 131 | 76 26 3.1:1 132 71 19 37 +1 134 92 36 2.6 :1 151 | 58 12 48:1 170 | 78 10 78-1 171 63 15 | 4.5 :1 77. 129 35 | 3.6 :1 | 1,405 450 | Average ratio of winged to apterous, 3.12:1. An analysis of the F, is not essential here and is omitted for the sake of brevity. It may be derived from the F, formulæ. Table II indicates the expected classes and ratios in F, and gives the actual numbers obtained in cul- ture No. 59, in which each class was recorded separately. In subsequent cultures of this experiment no attempt was made to separate any but the winged and apterous classes. Counts of the latter are given in Table III. 680 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII TABLE II MATING 59 Fz — Expected Classes Expected Ratio Aotus Ralio Actual No. TOUR Va Vy aA RS | 12 138:1 185 Lone VOUS Ooo Ses Oa eee ee i 3 4.251 57 HW WHILE OW is. 3s ere. 3 24:1 31 Minintiire vor: ol ee oo ve ens 3 eee 26 Miniature white oi cee eee | 3 Ari 53 Aptirons vor. 9.60. ee as sk | 4 Saek 44 Apterous verm. Goose veecccceessves | 2 1221 15 ADCOPOUR WHITE: OS oe ask sa ee es 2 a ey : ane TABLE III F, FROM MASS CULTURES Calture Wo, | Winged’ | -Apearouk Ratio 58 367 78 4:7 onl 59 352 3 By Beso | 96 556 114 48:1 7 306 70 Ao 21 120 597 103 5.08: 1 135 554 104 ES 203 137 405 49 6 Si 298 6:6 $1 157 405 53 16.2 3,840 699 Average ratio, 5.5 : 1. F, FROM PAIRS Culture No. Winged Apterous Ratio 155 141 35 AL Sk 156 38 11 G4 2:3 179 46 Average ratio, 3.9 : 1. F, FROM PAIRS Culture No. Winged Apterous Ratio 160 119 25 4.7531 162 123 27 46:1 172 266 62 CS: eau | 173 87 26 SA st 174 165 37 45:1 175 167 42 4: 1 176 171 46 SF BL 77 129. 8.0: 1 178 92 23 A tl 1,319 323 Average ratio, 4.08: 1 179 46 1,319 323 Total from pairs, 1,498 369 Ratio, 4.6 : 1. No. 575] AN APTEROUS DROSOPHILA 68 | It will be noted that the apterous classes fall a little below the expected numbers in most cases. This is char- acteristic of all weak races of Drosophila, and is doubtless due to the inability of some flies to mature. Of the winged classes the first two and the fifth exceed the Mendelian ratios, while the other two fall short, due to linkage be- tween white, vermilion and miniature.’ The distribution of apterous, however, is entirely independent of the others, showing that the factor responsible for it is not a member of the group containing those responsible for vermilion eyes, white eyes, or miniature wings. The ratio of winged to apterous in this particular cul- ture is 4.2:1. Table III includes a summary of this and nineteen similar cultures in which the parents were all descendants of the long-winged, vermilion-eyed female by the apterous male mentioned above. The first nine are mass cultures, the next two are pairs, from F, flies. Below these are offspring from nine pairs of F, flies. It is noticeable that the ratio of apterous to winged is greater in cultures where pairs are used than in mass cul- tures, though all parents in the latter are heterozygous. This, I believe, is unquestionably due to the low viability of the apterous flies, which prevents some of them from maturing in cultures where the competition is severe. For this reason the averages are given separately for pairs and for mass cultures. The average from pairs is 4.06: 1, while that from mass cultures is 5.5:1. This low viability is also shown by pairs, if the food conditions are not good, or if the culture becomes very dry. Experiment III.—To determine the relation between apterous and characters in Group ITI. It is obvious from Experiments I and II that apterous is not a sex-linked character (Group I). The present ex- periment is for the purpose of determining its relation to characters of Group III. As a representative of the latter group pink eye color was chosen. The results of the cross between this and apterous may be passed over briefly 5 For discussion of linkage between these characters see Morgan, 1911. 682 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII since they are similar to previous results in showing no linkage. A winged, pink-eyed male bred to an apterous, vermilion-eyed female (from Experiment III) gave, as expected, winged, vermilion-eyed males and winged, red- eyed females in F,. These inbred gave four classes of winged and four classes of apterous, i. e., red, vermilion, pink and orange.® The ratios are such as to show inde- pendent segregation of apterous and pink. Below is a summary of the expected and actual results. P, Apterous, vermilion 2 apvPX-avPX, Winged, pink ¢ ApV pX—App. F, All winged. Red 9 apvPX—ApV >X, Verm. 5 adpvPX—App. Eight kinds of eggs and four kinds of spermatozoa are formed by these F, flies, giving, through random fertiliza- tion, 32 classes of oe divided into eight groups, as shown i in oe TABLE IV F Expected Results Actual Results in Experiments te Classes | Ratio | 627 | 6287 | 629 | 630 | 681 | Total baer COS oi cee ic: 9 85 | 402 inged, vermilion.. >... gie ieg] S | sa n ne = | waar DINE Ee 4 3 25 | 4 | 111 Winged, orange......... gf" 110s" | , pO : | Apterous, rod. io. 004 2.3 3 22 | | 114 pterous, vermilion...... 3 . 13 a | a pa D i bic. Apterous, pink.......... 1 T 7 34 Apterous, orange........ 1 1 8 15 | 9 i9 7 In the table red and vermilion have been considered together as one class, because they both contain P; and similarly pink and orange have been considered together because they both contain p. The total numbers for the four classes give the ratios 402:111:114:34, or 10.8:3.6: 3.35:1,—a sufficiently close approximation to the expected 6 Orange eye is the double recessive pv-pv. : 7 In this culture the ratios are seen to diverge widely from the expected, due, I believe, to the poor cultural conditions in this case which prevented _ some of the weaker pink and orange flies from maturing No. 575] AN APTEROUS DROSOPHILA 683 9:3:3:1. These results clearly show the independence (i. e., lack of linkage) of apterous and pink. Table V includes all matings (giving apterous) in Ex- periment III, for the purpose of showing the ratio of winged to apterous. TABLE V Mating No. Winged | Apterous | Ratio 627 178 50 | 3.56 : 1 628 37 15 2.47:1 629 158 42 | 8.7 :1 630 46 13 3.63 : 1 631 94 28 3.326: 1 646 48 10 | 4.5.42 649 81 27 Soko S 650 44 14 3,15: 1 651 124 37 3.35 : 1 652 92 34 ay ae | GA 50 | 19 2.63: 1 692 75 | 27 3.15. 1 1,027 | w eee Average ratio winged to apterous, 3.25:1. Experiment IV.—To determine the relation between apterous and characters in Group II. The mutant called ‘‘black’’? (having black body and wings) was used in this experiment as a representative of Group II. No direct matings with apterous individuals, such as obtained in the three preceding cases, could be effected here, and consequently the winged brothers and sisters of apterous had to be used for crossing with black, according to the method described in the introduction. Matings of this kind (in pairs) gave, in F,, winged flies, some of which were heterozygous for apterous and black. These inbred (also in pairs) gave winged and apterous, and gray and black, as shown below: P, Black ¢ homozygous for wings Apb—Apb, Gray 9 heterozygous for apterous A,B-apB. F, Winged, heterozygous for black A yb—ApB, Winged, heterozygous for black and apterous A:b-%B. Only pairs in which both members were of the second type (4,b—a»B,—heterozygous for apterous) could pro- 684 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVII duce apterous. The others, therefore, are ignored. Con- sidering the second type alone, the analysis becomes: F, gametes (expected) A,b—A,B-apb—apB, A po—A p B —p b-a B š F, Expected classes. apB-apB apterous, gray. a>B-apb apterous (heterozygous for black). B-B winged, gray. aB-Ayb winged, heterozygous for black. apb-arpB apterous, heterozygous for black. apb-@b - apterous, black. ayb—ApB winged, heterozygous for black. apb—Apb winged, black. A,B-aB winged, gray. A,B-ayb winged, heterozygous for black. A,B-A,B winged, gray. - A,B-A,b winged, heterozygous for black. Arb-apB do. Apb-apb winged, black. A,b-A,B winged, heterozygous for black. Ayb-Apb winged, black. Expected ratios: 3 winged black; 6 winged heterozygous for black; 3 winged gray; 1 apterous black; 2 apterous heterozygous for black; 1 apterous gray. Actual results: winged black, winged heterozygous for black, and apterous gray, as shown in Table VI. In the last two matings black and heterozygous off- spring were counted as one class. Total: winged 701; apterous 174 or 4.02:1, The expectation for the F, if Ap and B segregate inde- pendently is equal numbers of black and gray among the winged and among the apterous offspring. Actually, how- ever, the apterous flies are all gray, and the winged flies are all black or heterozygous for black. Furthermore, the No. 575] AN APTEROUS DROSOPHILA 685 ratio of heterozygotes to pure blacks in the winged class shows that the flies which should have been gray accord- ing to expectation have been added to the heterozygotes. Likewise the gray flies in the apterous class are about four times as numerous as anticipated, showing that the ex- pected heterozygotes and blacks are here gray. From this it is evident that the factors a and B, on the one hand, and A» and b, on the other, have remained associated in the combination which they formed in the parents, instead of independently segregating. Such an explanation ac- counts for the absence of A,B and apb gametes in the F, generation, and consequently for the absence of gray, winged flies, and of black or heterozygous apterous flies in F,. The evidence accords with that obtained for many other mutant characters in Drosophila, and the explana- tion is the same as that given for the previous cases (e. g., Morgan, 1911, 1912; Morgan and Lynch; Sturtevant, 1913 a and b; Dexter). TABLE VI ACTUAL RESULTS ete pm tee Witt ‘Bee Apterous SS Mating | ER | No. | Gray Black | Heterozygous | Gray | Black | Heterozygous w i Bi A TBD 0 Logie s 22 | 71 s i 0 0 78a ee  20. | 54 15 G4 0 i o W 29 | 59 15 a 0 741 0 19 | 50 19 ae 0 745 0 35 86 30 oo 0 746 0 15 42 13 OY 0 | ae ak l oN | —____#.__* 725 53 14 734 79 24 The presence of a definite linkage or association be- tween apterous and black (i. e., between either ap or Ap and b or B depending upon the nature of the cross) as shown by this experiment, together with the absence of any such linkage with characters in Groups I and ITI, as shown by the preceding experiments, indicate that apter- 686 _ THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII ous is a member of Group II and is, presumably, asso- ciated or linked with all other characters belonging to that group. Supposedly this association of the characters results from an association of the factors responsible for them in the germ cells. And this latter association has been explained upon the assumption that factors responsible for linked characters are located in the same chromosome. The hypothesis has even been carried so far as to postulate a linear arrangement of the factors within the chromo- some—the relative position of the factors being deter- mined by the degree or amount of linkage existing between them. This conception and the data upon which it is based have been amplified by Morgan and by Sturtevant, and need not be dwelt on here. In the present case no attempt has been made to ascertain the exact degree of linkage between apterous and other characters in the group, except black, because of the difficulty of breeding the apterous flies. Apy tly the linkage between apter- ous and black is very close, if not complete, since no case of ‘‘crossing over” was observed among the 875 F, off- spring in this experiment (Table VI). There is a possi- bility that the classification of the F, apterous flies as all gray is not absolutely correct, because, owing to the diffi- culty of distinguishing gray from heterozygous black in apterous specimens, an occasional heterozygous fly might have passed for pure gray. However, if there had been any appreciable number of cross-overs in this direction, there would also have been some in the opposite direction, which fact would have been indicated by the presence of winged, gray flies. And since none of these were observed, it is safe to conclude that few or no cross-overs occurred, and hence that apterous is very closely, if not completely, linked to black. Experiment V.—To determine the relation between the apterous mutant and the ‘‘vestigial’’ mutant. Among the mutant characters of Group TI is one called ‘‘vestigial wing.” Flies having this character are more No. 575] AN APTEROUS DROSOPHILA 687 like the apterous individuals than are any of the other mutants, and since the two characters belong to the same group the question arose as to whether or not the factor responsible for one might be simply a modification of that responsible for the other. Experiment V was performed to determine this point. Long-winged flies heterozygous for apterous were crossed with vestigial winged individuals, and F, and F, generations raised. The F, flies were all long winged, which fact in itself indicates the independence of the two characters, for if they were allelomorphs either apterous or vestigial should have appeared. In F, both vestigial and apterous, as well as long-winged, flies appeared, showing conclusively the independence of the two characters. Summary OF EXPERIMENTS Experiments I and II show that the apterous character is a simple Mendelian recessive, which independently mendelizes with miniature wings, white eyes and ver- milion eyes, and hence is not sex- linked (i. e., not a mem- ber of Group I). Experiment III shows that the apterous factor is trans- mitted independently of the factor for pink eye, thus indi- cating that apterous is not a member of Group III. Experiment IV shows a linkage ratio to result from crosses involving apterous and black, the ratio being such as to indicate a very close linkage between apterous and black, and to identify apterous as a member of Group II. Experiment V shows apterous to be distinct from ves- tigial wing, to which it bears a considerable degree of resemblance. ORIGIN AND CHARACTERISTICS OF THE APTEROUS MUTANT The description of the apterous fly has been deferred up to this point in order that it might be combined with a discussion of the experimental results. The mutant has appeared upon several occasions, but 688 - THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (VoL. XLVIII always in the same stock (miniature white), and always with the same evidence of weakness and low viability. Fig. 1 is a camera drawing of a typical specimen, made by Miss E. M. Wallace. In morphological characters the fly differs from the normal in being entirely destitute of wings and in possessing greatly re- duced balancers. Likewise in Ra panos pe or physiological characters it devi- ates strikingly from the normal. This is best shown by means of a comparison between apterous flies and normal flies from which the wings have been removed. The latter are not appreciably inconvenienced by their loss of wings; they show char- acteristic vigor in their active running and jumping movements, they easily right themselves if overturned, or extricate themselves if entangled in food or cotton, and they are long lived and breed as prolifically as do winged individuals. In fact they show no ill effects except the in- ability to fly. The true apterous individuals, on the other hand, show marked abnormalities in all these respects. In- stead of being vigorous and active they are weak and usually sluggish; if overturned they have great difficulty in right- ing themselves; or, if entangled in food or cotton, they are usually unable to extricate themselves and consequently perish. Moreover, they are always short lived, even when kept under the best possible conditions and prevented from becoming entangled in food or cotton. And lastly they exhibit a most marked inability to breed, as noted in the experiments. This characteristic, as has been mentioned above, is so marked that the apterous flies were at first thought to be sterile. I am convinced now, however, that the difficulty is not one of sterility at all, but is due to a physical weakness which makes it extremely difficult for | the flies to copulate, and for the females, even when fertil- ized, to produce and lay eggs. Cytological examination has shown that the males produce spermatozoa in an ap- parently normal manner, yet prolonged observation of No. 575] AN APTEROUS DROSOPHILA 689 the flies has not revealed a single copulation or attempt at copulation on the part of an apterous male.’ Similarly the females have been shown to produce rudimentary eggs in an apparently normal manner, and in two cases females have produced offspring when fertilized by winged males, thus indicating their fertility. But many other cases have been observed in which apterous females were fertil- ized by winged males (or at least in which copulation took place), and yet in these observed cases the females invari- ably died without producing offspring,® because, I believe, of their physical weakness. From these facts it appears practically certain that the apparent sterility is not due to infertility of either sperm or eggs, but results from a weakness which makes it very difficult for the apterous flies to perform the reproductive processes. This explains why no crosses have been secured between apterous and apterous, although each sex has been suc- cessfully crossed to winged. It is also supported by the fact that from the cross between apterous male and winged female a large number of offspring were secured, since the winged female could produce many eggs,— whereas in the two crosses between apterous females and winged males only a very few offspring were secured, because the apterous females could only produce a few eggs. When the experiments were first begun it was hoped that sooner or later one or more inherently vigorous apterous flies would appear which might give rise to a vigorous race. But nothing of the sort took place, although numbers of the apterous flies were given oppor- tunity to breed all through the course of the experiments. Obviously, then, the physiological characteristics, as 8 Copulation in normal flies can be observed with very little difficulty. It is evident that at least one case of copulation by an apterous male occurred, namely in Experiment II. 9 Judging from the cases observed a large number (probably one spara _ Oor more) of apterous females must have been fertilized by win winged during the course of these experiments, yet only three of these gave ue. 690 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII shown by vigor and viability, are directly associated with morphological characters and are not to be separated from them by selection. In other words, the ‘‘factor’’ respon- sible for lack of wings is also responsible for physiological disturbances. The only suggestion of an inherent difference between different races, or strains of apterous, is the slight differ- ence in the percentage of apterous offspring in Experi- ments I and III as compared with II and IV. In I and IIT the ratios of winged to apterous are 3.12:1 and 3.24:1, respectively, while in Experiments II and IV they are 4.06:1 and 4.02:1. This deviation is not great, but it is fairly constant, and is sufficient, I believe, to indicate a real difference. But whether it is to be explained upon the assumption that in I and III the apterous parents were inherently stronger than in II and IV is not so clear. It might equally well be explained upon the basis of differ- ences in the winged races to which apterous was crossed. Unfortunately, an experimental analysis of the question is prohibited by the difficulty of breeding the apterous flies, and it must, therefore, be left open. One fact, however, is clear, namely that there is no progressive increase in viability of the apterous flies, for the apterous parent in Experiment II, where the viability appears to be low, was descended directly from that in I where it appears to be high, and likewise the parent from the apterous side in IV was obtained directly from III. In conclusion it may be profitable to call to mind briefly the bearing of certain of the above data on the question of the nature and behavior of Mendelian ‘*factors.’’ The present case of a definite correlation between lack of wings, reduction in size of balancers, and weak physical constitution in the apterous race of Drosophila, shows clearly that one factor may have far reaching effects, and not be limited to any particular part or organ,—a fact No. 575] AN APTEROUS DROSOPHILA 691 which has been long known, and often mentioned,!° but by no means universally recognized. Correlated with, or resulting from this principle is the conception that the final result of ontogenetic develop- ment is not due to the independent action of various factors and their products, but is due to the combined action, or the interaction of these products during devel- opment. To illustrate by the wing of a fly,—it is probable that the normal development of such an organ is not dependent solely upon one factor, but that it is influenced by many factors. This is strongly suggested by data derived from the various wing mutations in Drosophila. These have dealt with a large number of factors, each of which is responsible for a definite wing modification. For instance, one factor is responsible for miniature wings, another for vestigial, another for rudimentary, another for curved, ete.!!_ From the fact that these mutant factors (which may be considered as modifications of factors in the normal fly) influence the wings, it seems highly prob- able that their normal allelomorphs also influence wing production in the wild fly. Finally I wish to thank Dr. T. H. Morgan for kindly assistance and advice in connection with this work. BIBLIOGRAPHY Dexter, John S. 1912 a Coupling of Certain Sex-linked Characters in Drosophila. iol. Bull., Vol. 23, p. 183. Morgan, T. ra 1911. An Attempt to Analyze the Constitution of the Chromosomes on the Basis of Sex-limited Inheritance in Drosophila, Jour. Exp. ool., Vol. 2, p. 365. 1912a, Bight Factors that Show Sex-linked Inheritance in Drosophila. ence, N. S., Vol. 35, p. 472. 10 Most recently, perhaps, by Morgan (1913a, page 9): ‘‘A change in a factor may have far-reaching consequences. Every part of the organism capable of reacting to the new change is affected. Though we seize upon th most conspicuous difference between the old type and its mutant, and make use of this alone, every student of heredity is familiar with cases where more than the part taken as the index is affected. Weismann’s theory, on the other hand, seems to identify each character with a special determinant ...’’ 11 The same is true for various eye colors, and body colors 692 . 1912b. 1912c. 1912d. 1913a. 1913b, Morgan, T. 1912. Morgan, T. 1913 Sturtevant, 19134 19136. THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [VoL. XLVIII A Modification of the Sex Ratio, and of Other Ratios, in Drosophila through Linkage. Zeit. f. ind. Abst. u. Vererb., Bd. 7, p. 323. Heredity of Body Color in Drosophila. Jour. Exp. Zool., Vol. » p. 27. The Explanation of a New Sex-ratio in Drosophila and Com- plete Linkage in the Second Chromosome of the Male. Science, N. B., Vol. 36, p: 718 Factors and Unit Characters in Mendelian Heredity. AMER. Nart., Vol. 47, p Simplicity versus Adequacy in Mendelian Formule. AMER. 72. H., and C. J. Lynch. The Linkage of Two Factors in oe that Are Not Sex- linked. Biot. Bull., Vol. 23, p. 1 H., and E. Catt ell. Additional Data for the Study of FE oat Inheritance in Drosophila. Jour. Exp. Zool., Vol. A. H. The Linear Arrangement of Six Sex-linked Factors in Drosophila, as Shown by their Mode of Association. Jour. ap. Zool., Vol. 14, p. 43. A Third Group of Linked Genes in Drosophila ampelophila. Science, N. S., Vol. 37, p. 990. SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION FORMULA FOR THE RESULTS OF INBREEDING IN connection with Pearl’s recent valuable analyses of the results of inbreeding (1, 2, 3), a comparison of these results with those from self-fertilization is of interest. In my note on the latter (4), I gave a formula for the rate at which organisms become homozygotie through continued self-fertilization. This occurs more slowly in the various types of inbreeding, but Pearl gives no general formula for it. For purposes of comparison I have worked out from Pearl’s data the general formula for the rate at which organisms become homozygotic through continued brother by sister mating; as such formule appear to be of perma- nent value, it is here given.* What the formula gives is, pre- cisely, (1) the proportion of individuals that will be homozygotie or any given character after any number of unbroken genera- tions of such inbreeding, (2) the average proportion of the char- acters of a given individual that will be homozygotie after any number of unbroken generations of such inbreeding. The nu- merical value so obtained may conveniently be called the co- efficient of homozygosis. The formula turns out to be a combination of the successive powers of 2, with the successive terms of the Fibonacci series, which appears in so curious a way in various natural phenomena. In this series every term is the sum of the two preceding terms, the series beginning: 0, 1, 1, 2, 3, 5, 8, 13, ete. Let «=the coefficient of homozygosis. n= the number of inbred generations (the number of times successive brother by sister mating has occurred). fis fo» fa, ete., = the successive terms of the Fibonacci series üs f0, f= 1, ate). Then the formula for the coefficient of homozygosis is: w Eh aha . ete, o 2 y — L = (The terms in the numerator are continued until the exponent of 2 becomes 1 In conversation, Dr. Pearl urged the publication of the present note, otherwise I should not at this time have dealt with a matter which he has under analysis. 693 694 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [VoL. XLVIII Thus, if the number of inbreedings (n) is 1. 99 t= z= 1/2, or 50 per cent. If n= 4 3 2 1 0 pest ce i TAPE At Ab én 68.15 par eat If n=9 28-4027 + 1,29 4 1.25 4 2.244 3.93 4 5.9? +. 8.914 13.29 ia 53 ).2? + 8.2" + 13. Yr A == 457/512, or 89.26 per cent. It n= 16 x or 97.38 per cent. _ 63819 65536 As these examples show, the formula gives the results that were obtained by Pearl in the detailed working out (so far as this was carried), as given in Pearl’s table I (2, p. 62). (It will be noted that Pearl counts as generation 1 the one before inbreed- ing has occurred, so that his generation 10, for example, is that in which there have been 9 inbreedings (n=9). If one is working out the values of the coefficient x for a series of generations, the above formula may be expressed as a simple rule, applicable after the value for »—1 is obtained. This e is: The value of the coefficient of homozygosis x for any term (as the nth) is obtained by doubling the numerator and denominator of the fraction expressing the value for the previous term, and adding to the numerator the corresponding (n— 1th) term of the Fibonacci series. Or, in view of the peculiar nature of the Fibonacci series, the rule may be expressed as follows: Double the numerator and denominator. and add to the nu- merator the sum of the last two numbers so added. Thus, since x for 1 inbreeding = 1/2 2x1+0 ?? 2? en aetna nee were nti 2” 2 =F = 3/4 2x2+1 79) 2? (ec ee ee, x 3 eve a 5/8 » ar 2K5+1._ z 4 O Ga == 11/16, ete. No. 575] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 695 After obtaining x, or the proportion of homozygotes for any one pair of characters, the proportion y for any number m of pairs is obtained simply by raising x to the mth power, that is: ya, Thus, after two generations of brother X sister mating, the proportion of homozygotes for three pairs of characters is (1/2)*==1/8, or 12.5 per cent. After 8 generations of such inbreeding the proportion homozygotie for 10 pairs of char- acters is: 10 (555) = 24.05 per cent. The corresponding value in the case of continued self-fertili- zation is 99.61 per cent. (4, p. 491). Whether it may be possible to obtain a similar formula for the coefficient of homozygosis in the cases of mating of cousin X cousin or of parent X offspring, remains to be discovered. Pearl’s ‘‘coefficient of inbreeding’’ gives the percentage of lacking ancestors in a given pedigree, as compared with the number that would be present if all the parents were unrelated. In order to compare self-fertilization with inbreeding in this respect, Pearl’s formulæ for the coefficient of inbreeding may be expressed in terms of the number of successive inbreedings (7) ; for many purposes the formule appear more convenient so ex: pressed. The following gives these formule for self-fertilization and the three types of inbreeding, together with those, so far as worked out, for the proportion of individuals homozygotie with respect to a given character. In all these, n is the number of successive self-fertilizaticus or inbreedings. Coefficient of Inbreeding. Coefficient of Homozygosis. 2" —1 Self-fertilization 7 = Qn Qn-1 + f,-2"-2-+ fa 27. o -etc. Qn Brother x Sister aR 2" eire Omala x Coada oa ? a E E ? Parent x Offspring ree ~ It will be observed that in self-fertilization the value of the coefficient of inbreeding is, curiously, the same as that of the coefficient of homozygosis, while in the other cases there is no evident simple relation between the two. Further, the coefficient 696 ` THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. XLVII of inbreeding in brother X sister mating is the same as for self- fertilization, save that it lags one generation behind the latter; thus the coefficient for the fourth generation of self-fertilization is the same as that for the fifth of brother X sister mating. Pearl (1, p. 592) has already pointed out that in cousin mating the coefficient is one-half that for brother X sister, with a lag of one generation; as compared with self-fertilization the lag is two generations. No such simple relation is apparent between the proportions of homozygotes resulting from the diverse methods of breeding, though possibly such may yet be discovered. H. S. JENNINGS PAPERS CITED 1. Pearl, R. A contribution toward an analysis of the problem of inbreed- ing. This JOURNAL, XLVII, October, 1913, pp. 577-614. ——. On the results of inbreeding a Mendelian population; a correction and extension 7 previous conclusions, This JOURNAL, XLVIII, Jan- uary, 1914, pp. 57-62 On a pace formula for the constitution of the nth generation vf a Mendelian population in which all matings are of brother X sister. This JOURNAL, XLVIII, August, 1914, pp. 491-494 4 Jennings, H. S. Production of pure homozygotie organisms from hetero- zygotes by self-fertilization. -This JOURNAL, XLVI, August, 1912, pp. 487—491. . A SHORT-CUT IN THE COMPUTATION OF CERTAIN PROBABLE ERRORS In his handbook of statistical methods, on p. 38, Dr. C. B. Davenport! gives a short method for the calculation of the prob- able errors of some of the commonest statistical constants, in a table of logarithmic formule. It would seem that the simple and obvious short-cut involved has not been given the attention it deserves in connection with non-logarithmie calculation. The logarithmic formule are as follows :? (1) log E, = log .6745 + log ø — $ log n [since E, = 6745 =|. log E, = log E, — 4 log 2 [since E, = .6745 T or, B, =~E,~+ |, 1 Davenport, C. B., ‘‘Statistical Methods with Special ae to Bio- logical Variation,’’ 2d ed., 1904, New York, John Wiley & s. 2 A indicates the weighted arithmetic mean, o the standard ‘dative. and -Oth coefficient of variability. (2) No. 575] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 697 (3)8 log Ee = log E, — log A [since E, = E, + A]. Now, if one is working with a calculating machine, he can simply carry the value of E a to two or three more decimal places than are to be retained, and then divide by the square root of 2 to get E,; similarly, the latter’ value, divided. by the mean, gives Eo. The writer prefers, however, to caleulate the values in the ordinary w pe on the machine, using Miss Gibson’st table for 6745 , and then to use the short method in checking. guns ag The original computations can be indicated and performed with great confidence and rapidity, since it is hardly possible to make an error that will not be discovered in the checking.’ It is obviously safer, as well as much quicker, to check in this way than to repeat the original processes. Howarp B. Frost CITRUS EXPERIMENT STATION, RIVERSIDE, CAL, GALTON AND DISCONTINUITY IN VARIATION Ir seems not to be generally realized that Galton recognized both continuity and discontinuity, both in variation and inherit- ance. Of course, all biologists are familiar with ‘‘Galton’s poly- gon,’’ in which slight oscillations of the polygon on one of its faces, but without a change of face, are compared with ‘‘small unstable deviations’’ (fluctuations), while a larger oscillation, in which the polygon moves over to a new face, is compared to a sport ... of such marked peculiarity and stability as to rank as a new type, capable of becoming the origin of a new race with very little as- sistance on the part of natural selection.* Galton’s polygon illustrated for him how the following uaii tions may co-exist: (1) Variability within narrow limits without prejudice to the purity of the breed. (2) Partly stable sub-types. (3) Tendency, when much disturbed, to revert from a sub-type to an earlier form. (4) Oceasjonal— sports which may give rise to new types. These four types would seem to correspond rather well to what 3 Formula (3) gives, of course, the approximate or uncorrected value Ee, 4Gibson, Winifred, ‘¢Tables for Facilitating the Computation of Prob- able Errors,’’ Biometrika, 4: 385-393. 3 tables. 5 Unless, of course, one misreads the figures from the machine in checking. 1 Natural Inheritance, ’’ London, 1889, p. 28. 698 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII are now called (1) fluctuations or ‘‘non-inherited’’ (in reality, I think, partially inherited) continuous variations; (2) instability resulting from a heterozygous or partially heterozygous condi- tion; (3) reversions, now believed to result chiefly from cross- ing; and (4) mutations. Galton is equally explicit i in other statements on this subject. Like Darwin, he admitted the facts both of continuity and dis- continuity in variation; but, unlike Darwin, he also recognized discontinuity or E A as well as continuity or blending, in inheritance. Thus he says, in a paragraph headed ‘‘stability of sports’’ :? ae a does not show that those wide varieties which are called “sports ” are unstable. On the contrary, they are often transmitted to successive generations with curious persistence. Neither is there any reason for expecting otherwise. While we can well understand that a strained modification of a type would not be so stable as one that ap- proximates more nearly to the typical center, the variety may be so wide that it falls into different conditions of stability, and ceases to be a strained modification of the original type. In another paragraph,* headed ‘‘Evolution not by minute steps only,’’ he says: The theory of evolution might dispense with a restriction, for which it is diffieult to see either the need or the justification, namely, that the course of evolution always proceeds by steps that are severally minute, and that become effective only through accumulation. That the steps may be small and that they must be small are very different views; it is only to the latter that I object... . An apparent ground for the com- mon belief is founded on the fact that wherever search is made for in- termediate forms between widely divergent varieties, whether they be of plants or of animals, of weapons or utensils, of customs, religion of language, or of any other product of evolution, a long and orderly series can usually be made out, each member of which differs in an almost im- perceptible degree from the adjacent specimens. But it does not at all follow because these intermediate forms have been found to exist, that they are the very stages that were passed through in the course of evo- lution. Counter evidence exists in abundance, not-only of the appear- ance of considerable sports, but of their remarkable stability in hered- itary transmission, Again, Galton not only believed in the existence of both blended and alternative inheritance, but he recognized the im- 2 L. c., p. 30. sL ¢., p. 32. No. 575] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 699 ‘portance of the latter in connection with the survival of new races. Thus he writes The quadroon child of the mulatto and the white has a quarter tint; some of the children may be altogether darker or lighter than the rest, but they are not piebald. Skin-color is therefore a good example of what I call blended inheritance. . Next as regards heritages that come io aia from one progenitor to the exclusion of the rest. Eye-color is a fairly good illustration of Aa There are probably no heritages that perfectly blend or that abso- lutely exclude one another, but all heritages have a tendency in one or the other direction, and the tendency is often a very strong one. On the following page Galton remarks that A peculiar interest attaches itself to mutually exclusive heritages, owing to the aid they must afford to the establishment of incipient races. He thus recognizes the invalidity of Darwin’s objection to ‘‘single variations’’ as a factor in evolution, namely, that they would certainly be swamped by crossing with the general popu- lation. It would, therefore, appear that in his recognition of continu- ity as well as discontinuity both in variation and heredity, Galton was in advance of his time, and more in accord with some of the current views. R. RUGGLES GATES UNIVERSITY OF LONDON ` REPULSION IN MICE IN the February number of the AmERICAN NaruRaList Dr. C. Little criticizes the results of my mouse-breeding experiments which I published in the Zeitschrift für Induktive Abstam- mungs- und Vererbungs-lehre Bd. VI, Heft 3. The chief point, ‘on which he disagrees with me, is the interpretation of the results I obtained in breeding black and albino mice together. The fact is, that in my paper on mice, I overlooked a serious error. In three sentences on page 126, relating to test ATIR of albinos, the words ‘‘black’’ and ‘‘agouti’’ changed places. printed in the paper these sentences run: Without exception they have given black or equal numbers of black and albino young, depending upon the purity of the black used. But never has one of these albinos produced a single agouti young in a mat- ing with black. Counting together the colored young of such families I get 89 black ~~ iE api 5 Cates of kde in such crosses are of course now well- known. 700 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST ([Vou.XLVIII . These errors were corrected in an “‘errata’’ in Band VI, heft 5, which Dr. Little unhappily did not find. The sentences should read: Without exception they have given agouti, or equal numbers of agouti and albino young, depending upon the purity of the black used. But never has one of these albinos produced a single black young in a mating with black. Counting together the colored young of such families I get 89 agouti young. Professor Punnett was so kind as to draw my attention to these mistakes. They were corrected in the reprints sent out. The facts were simply these: Albinos were bred of two sorts, with and without @ (the gene which agoutis have more than blacks). These albinos can only be distinguished by test-mating them to blacks. The albinos with G (aG) give agouti young, if mated to black (Ag), the ag albinos give black young from such a-mating. In one series, some agoutis were produced, which were heterozygous for A as well as for G@(AaGq). Ordinarily, such agoutis, when mated inter se, produce 9 agouti (1 AAGG, 2 AAGg, 2 AaGG, 4 AaGg), 3 black (1 aaGG, 2 aaGg) and 4 albinos ( 1 aaGG, 2 aaGg, 1 aagg) in every sixteen. Mated to albinos without @(ag) the ordinary AaGg animals give four. kinds of young, agoutis (AaGg), blacks (Aagg) and two kinds of albinos (aaGg) and (aagg) in equal numbers. Now these particular AaGg animals did not produce four kinds of gametes, as expected, namely, AG, Ag, aG and ag, but only two kinds, Ag and aG. Thirty one agoutis were test-mated to aagg albinos. -These test matings gave 181 young, of which 94 were black (Aagg) and 87 albino (aaG@qg). No agoutis were produced. As a further proof, the result of breeding these agoutis inter se, can be adduced. These matings gave 73 agouti (AaGg), 37 black (AAgg) and 32 albinos (aaGG@). Of these 32 albinos, thirteen were tested by mating them to blacks. If one of them should have lacked G, it would have given black young. But no black young were produced. Some young were albino (when the black parent was heterozygous for A), but all the colored young were agouti (89 in all). — This, I hope, will make it perfectly clear, that in this series we have been dealing with a case of repulsion between the genes A and G. A. L. HAGEDOORN _ Bussum, HOLLAND No. 575] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 701 THE OSTEOLOGY OF A DOUBLE-HEADED CALF THROUGH the kindness of Mr. Charles O. Reed, taxidermist, of Fairmont, W. Va., the writer received the skulls and anterior cervical vertebrae of a double-headed calf which seemed of suffi- cient interest to warrant a brief description. According to Mr. Reed the calf’s mother was a four-year-old, thoroughbred Herford, living at Grafton, W. Va., owner not ‘mentioned. At her first labor this cow gave birth to twins, supposedly normal, though it was not so stated. The second calf was “‘slightly deformed,’’ but in what way Reed did not know. The third labor produced the double-headed calf in question, which was of unusual size, and was killed in parturition. According to Reed ‘‘This calf would have lived if it could have been brought through O. K.’’ He dissected it and found the ‘‘alimentary canal, blood vessels and trachea normal.’’ The bones in the occipital region are slightly broken, probably done in disarticulating the skulls from the neck; and in the left skull the left premaxilla was lost and was replaced by a roughly carved piece of wood for the sake of symmetry. In macerating the skulls, for the purpose of removing all the flesh, many of the loose sutures separated, and in gluing the bones together again it was not always possible to completely close the sutures. As may be seen in the figures there is a considerable though not very great difference in the size of the skulls, the right being the larger. They were detached from the cervical vertebre when received, but the photographs show their approximate position in relation to the neck and to each other. Each skull is twisted and bent away from the other, the bend being most marked just cephalad to the orbits. The left skull is the more distorted. . The articulation of the skulls with the fused atlas was so crude — that Reed, who had seen the skulls before disarticulation, had to be appealed to to decide which skull was right and which was left. Fig. 1 is a photograph of the dorsal aspect of the skulls and the first three cervical vertebre. The distortion of the two skulls is of about the same character but is, as noted above, more marked in the left skull. The parietal (p) is normal. The posterior regions of the fron- tals (f) are normal, but their anterior ends are bent laterally, 702 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII which causes a slight curvature in the sagittal suture. It is in the region of the lachrymals (1) that the distortion is most marked, so that the lachrymal on the convex side of the bend, especially Fig. 1. DORSAL VIEWS OF THE Two SKULLS AND OF THE First THREE CERVICAL VERTEBR®. MANDIBLES IN POSITION. a, atlas; aw, axis; e, extra bone between maxilla and premaxilla; f, frontai ; l, lachrymal; m, malar; mz, maxilla; n, sal; o, occipital; p, parietal; pm premaxilla. in the left skull, is much longer than that on the opposite side; the same is true of the malars (m), of the maxillaries (ma), and, to a less degree, of the premaxille (pm). The nasals (n) are also unsymmetrical, but do not differ much in size; they are simply, as a pair, pushed to the side. Fig. 2. The ventral aspect of the skull shows even greater ab- normalities than the dorsal. The occipital (0), as noted above, was somewhat injured by the person who disarticulated the skulls from the neck, but it is quite unsymmetrical, especially in its exoccipital region. In the left skull (right in this figure) all the other bones seen in this aspect are bent, but in the other skull most of the bones are comparatively straight. In the right skull a suture in front of the teeth separates off an extra bone (e) on each side, between the maxilla and the pre- maxilla, that of the right side being much the larger. In the left skull these extra bones are not present though a partial suture, No. 575] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 703 extending about half way through the left maxilla, is visible in this view of t ~— 1e skull. FIG. 2. VENTRAL VIEWS OF THE SKULLS AND THE neat Two CERVICAL VER TEBRÆ. iaoa REMO Fig. 3 shows the curious distortion of the mandibles, which seem to be bent in more or less the same direction. In the right man- dible the left half has four incisor teeth, the right half has three. In the left mandible the right half has four teeth, the left half has three, though one tooth is missing from each half. As noted above, the skulls, when received, were disconnected from the vertebre: but the latter, three in number, were strung together on a small piece of rope and presumably were the first three cervicals; they are shown in a dorsal view in Fig. 1, ven- tral view in Fig. 2. and anterior view in Fig. 3. The first of these is presumably a compound atlas (a) since it articulates with each of the skulls. though in a very crude way. It consists of eight loosely united elements which became completely sepa- glued together again. In the rated in cleaning and had to be bone from which dorsal view, Fig. 1, is seen a small, irregular radiate three somewhat symmetrical bones, the largest lying it the median plane between the bases of the skulls. This larger 704 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (VoL. XLVIII bone is pierced by two large foramina; each of the other two bones shows in this view a foramen which branches and opens both on the antero-median and the postero-lateral surfaces. The Fig. 3. DORSAL VIEWS OF THE MANDIBLES AND ANTERIOR VIEWS OF THE FIRST THREE CERVICAL VERTEBRÆ. ventral view, Fig. 2, shows a very irregular group of bones, the smallest of which is for articulation with the following vertebra. The second vertebra (ax), supposedly the axis, exhibits no indication of an odontoid process and articulates in a very crude way with the preceding bone. Its dorsal spine is rather elon- gated in an antero-posterior direction, but otherwise it bears no closer resemblance to an axis than to any other cervical vertebra. Its centrum was so loosely fused with the arch on either side it became detached in cleaning and had to be glued in place. The third vertebra exhibits no peculiarities that warrant de- scription. A. M. REESE. WEST VIRGINIA UNIVERSITY, MORGANTOWN VOL. XLIII, NO. 576 © DECEMBER, 1914 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST A MONTHLY JOURNAL Devoted to the Advancement of the Biological Sciences with Special Reference to the Factors of Evolution CONTENTS Page . The Failure of Ether to Produce Mutations in Drosophila. Professor T. H. MORGAN 705 bot The Analysis of a Case of Continuous Variation in — ad a Spats of its Linkage Relations. Professor JOHN S. DEXTER 71 Ill. Shorter Articles and Correspondence : On the Progressive Increase of Homo- zygous Brother-Sister Matings. H. prim- - - ~ = oo D soal TOS . NotesandLiterature: Mendelian Fluctuations: G.U.Y. pi a Index to Volume XLIII - s THE SCIENCE PRESS LANCASTER, PA. GARRISON, N. T. NEW YORK: SUB-STATION 84 The American Naturalist intended for pae t and books, etc., intended for review should be MSS sent to the Sec! of THE AMERICAN NATURAL Short articles containing summaries of res problems rod organic evolution are especially rlon. in ops: oe RALIST, Garrison- -on- agrbees = York. earch work bea on the and will be Sey afne an ndrea reprints of si f are supplied to authors free of charge. y Further ı Pariak will be supplied at c Subscript ons and p saaa aeina ae be sent to the Ponas. The subscription price is four dollars a year. oreign postage fty cents and Canadian postage twenty-five ak additional. The ppi for single poiri is forty cents. The advertising rates are Four Dollars for a THE SCIENCE PRESS NEW YORK: Sub-Station 84 Entered as second-class matter, April 2, 1908, at the Post Office at Lancaster, Pa., under the Act ot Congress of March 3, 1879. Lancaster, Pa. Garrison, N. Y. FOR SALE ARCTIC, ICELAND and GREENLAND BIRDS’ SKINS, Low Prices rticulars of G. DINESEN. ra Collecto Husavik, North Iceland, Via Leidie, enatand Well Skr JAPAN NATURAL HISTORY SPECIMENS Perfect Condition and Lowest Prices. Specialty: Bird Skins, Oology, Entomology, Marine Animals and others. Catalogue free. Correspond- ence solicited. T. FUKAI, Naturalist, Konosu, Saitama, Japan For Sale Entire An important collection of Indian Birds’ Eggs, containing approximately 3,300 specimens of about 620 species, and in- cluding among other varieties the Green- ish Willow Warbler (Acanthopneuste viridanus), Red-browed Finch (Calla- canthis burtoni), Tibetan Sand-Grouse (Syrrhaptes tibetanus), Spotted Sand- Grouse (Pteroclurus senegallus), Ibis- bill (Ibidorhynchus struthersi), White- bellied Minivet (Pericrocotus erythro- pygius), Bearded Vulture (Gypaëtus barbatus), numerous Cuckoos with their hosts, ete., ete. Particulars and full list may be had from W. F. H. ROSENBERG 57 Haverstock Hill London, N. W. | Marine Biological Laboratory | Woods Hole, Mass. INVESTIGATION Busilities for research in Zoology, ryology, Physiology and Bot- Entire Year a ty-six private labora- rar, $100 each for not over three y tables are avail- able for bataar in — who desire to work under the direction of aaa the. of the sale The fee for such a table is $50.0 Courses of labora ok n INSTRUCTION with E tenbared are fered i in Inverte- July—August plants n i ¢ ia embe ma mic stages: all s raae of Algae, Dr oor badi SUPPLY DEPARTMENT Open the Entire Year z for. the m Sasori oF furnished in season as ordered. Price li of we logical an $ oieniees TEk po on Ppr, estion À and all information ma address GEO. M. GRAY, Curator, Woods Hole, Mass The annual announcement will be sent on application nt The Director, Marine Biological Laboratory, Woods Hole Mass- THE AMERICAN NATURALIST Vout. XLVIII December 1914 No. 576 THE FAILURE OF ETHER TO PRODUCE MUTA- TIONS IN DROSOPHILA PROFESSOR T. H. MORGAN COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY THE many mutants of Drosophila ampelophila that have appeared ‘‘under domestication’’ have raised the question as to the cause or causes that have brought about the re- sult. Since every fly that has passed through our hands has been etherized once in its life, usually before it begins to lay its eggs if a female or before mating if a male, it might appear that this recurring condition was respon- sible for the mutations. At any rate it seemed worth while to put this view to a test, if for no other reason than to remove from one’s mind the suspicion that ether UGIL. Preliminary trials showed that two drops of ether (on a piece of cotton). in a quart milk bottle, tightly stoppered with a cotton plug, would not noticeably affect the flies in half an hour, three drops made them slightly ‘‘stupid,’’ four drops more so, and five drops quieted them. It was found that they would for the most part recover even after 6, 7 and 8 drops of ether. If etherized twice daily the flies were so far weakened that they generally died without laying any eggs. Therefore in the later experi- ments the flies were etherized only once a day or once in two days. The larve (beginning two days after the eggs were laid, 705 706 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII at which time the eggs have hatched) can stand more ether. Twice daily throughout their larval and pupal lives (approximately 11 days) 6, or 7, or 8, or 9 or even 10 drops of ether were added to the quart bottles. The ether excited the larve at first, then quieted them; later they re- covered. In such tests the larve were kept almost con- tinuously in an atmosphere of ether from birth to emer- gence of the fly and in a few cases the etherization was con- tinued with the flies also. By covering a wide range of stages and conditions I hoped to find the critical point, if any such existed, when ether would act. Since, as the sequel will show, no specific results were obtained it seems unnecessary to give the details of all these trials. Double, and in one ease triple recessive, stocks were used for the work, because experience had already shown that even with great care contamination may occur. One or two flies that came from escaped mutants would ruin the value of the data, but the operator can protect himself by using stocks that have already two or more recessive char- acters. If such flies mutate in one of the characters in- volved the presence of the other one will make it certain that the mutant belonged to this culture, and had not come in from outside; if a change appeared in some other part, the double recessive character would still identify the stock. Two of the stocks used had sex-linked characters, i. e., eosin miniature and cherry club vermilion. If a mutation should appear that involved these characters it would become evident at once in the male offspring; for, the male gets his single sex chromosome from his mother and exhibits her sex-linked factors. Of course this would be equally true for any other sex-linked char- acter that appeared, but in practise it is impossible to thoroughly examine each fly in every possible part, so that I had to confine my attention to certain organs, and in these cases I concentrated on the mutant characters. Con- spicuous mutations in other parts would, I think, have been picked up, but minor ones would probably have been missed. On the other hand, if changes taking place in No. 576] MUTATIONS IN DROSOPHILA Pita! the chromosomal material are the basis for mutation it would seem perhaps a priori unlikely that the same changes should occur at the same time in both members of a pair, and if not the effect would not appear in the next generation, and not until two flies of the later progeny each carrying one mutant factor met. Whatever weight may be attached to this argument—we know really nothing as to the origin of mutations—it seemed necessary to carry some stocks to another generation; and this was done. The following are the totals of offspring produced by flies from larve that had been etherized twice daily from the time of hatching to the winged state: Bhk væk r oesG voc es eae E GEE T io Puk Dak oes. E RE e ES T AE A a bees 1,390 Ronm DOMINARE soo a oot aa aes cK ee 871 Cherry olub Vermilion 05.0000. is peace es sis orria 364 Pink ebòùy SORIG s 6205 5a Wis ee eee oes 1,311 ital o ie ee kes 4,802 In the next case fewer drops of ether were used—four or five throughout larval and pupal life. The parent flies were changed to new bottles quite often to prevent crowd- ing and abundant food was supplied: Black vestigial .........0cscececeer cesses ceeeeeees 2,122 Pink HACK ay oss es ae ws EVER Bens Fomine eos Vee oe be 6,762 Hoin miniature... vs cs 6 ce erases ees hese toe pes oes 2,603 Pink ebony sepia ....... esses cee reereetenereecess 953 12,440 T reo Ss es ewe ive we be OE CS ee oe The following data are from the offspring of the flies that had 8 and 10 drops of ether twice daily: Pink blak oc Fo ee a wk ee areen 3,440 i i 2,775 Fosin miniature 2. ..cccc cece ccs ret ss eects es eera 2, a rs ae cet hee ae re 6,215 The next data are the records of the offspring of 708 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII adult flies that had been etherized several times (usually twice a day) just after they had hatched: Dinek Voutimia) (6! CGN) ies is esi ae Ves e es 3g koe 870 Black vestigial (V times} Soares ie ses wie sates 143 Black vestigial (8 times) ................ sey he aa Ea 694 Toró Tame (S See ee a ie ce kee ee 81 Eons mimiature: (3 UMA) s. asair os 5 es a 206 Mosin miniature (O°: Wee os re e eh 428 Cherry club vermillion (8 times) > roii. cereri dirr 713 Cherry club vermillion (8 times) .............-2500%5 476 FONE ae yao OW a ikia Secu ee roles ro 3,611 Finally some of the flies that had appeared, in the ex- periments in which 8 and 10 drops of ether had been used throughout the larval and pupal life, were bred and gave in the next generation the following records: PUR NOR oo E eens a Sha ae ase ee hone sui 2,186 MOOT) DIOR ONG ks bk sy wicca bh oe ON a a Carry CBD VADANO icy ee cake hron KASA eh ees 709 PER MOUS BOER os i ins ks as ECA eS RS ee 539 ae EN a aera Oe pe car grater Grain ie ata gine 4,100 In a grand total of 31,168 flies subjected to ether, there was not a single mutation observed. It seems highly probable therefore that ether has no specific effect in pro- ducing mutations in Drosophila ampelophila. It might, of course, still be said that mutations are so rare, that, although caused by ether, they still are not frequently enough produced to make 31,000 flies a sufficient guaran- tee. Granting this, it still remains that since no mutants appeared under this excessive treatment, ether does not play the rôle of a specific agent causing the. mutations of Drosophila, and one is inclined to look elsewhere for a solution of the problem. One of the first mutants that I observed in ampelophila appeared in the offspring of flies that had been treated with radium and although there was no proof that the radium had had a specific effect I felt obliged to state the actual case, refraining carefully from any statement of No. 576] MUTATIONS IN DROSOPHILA 709 causal connection.! Nevertheless, I have been quoted as having produced the first mutants by the use of radium. I may add that repetition of the experiment on a large scale both with the emanations of an X-ray machine and from radium salts has failed to produce any mutations, although the flies were made sterile for a time. Loeb and Bancroft also tried the effect of radium.? They found a black mutant type after treatment with radium but since the same type appeared in the control they do not believe that its appearance had any connection with the radium. They also state that after treatment a white-eyed female appeared in the first generation, and suggest that a white eyed male may have existed in a previous generation that escaped notice, but if it had been found in a previous gen- eration, the mutation or the contamination must have been earlier than the one that produced the white-eyed female; for, a white-eyed male takes two generations to reappear again. Pink-eyed flies were also found both in the control and in the treated flies. In regard to another mutant type, they state: We succeeded in producing short winged specimens in two different cultures by treating them with radium, while thus far we have not observed this mutation in cultures not treated with radium. But although ‘‘two hundred different cultures’’ were subsequently treated with radium and no short-winged (miniature) flies appeared, I get the impression that Bancroft and Loeb must have had stock that was already contaminated by some recessive mutant factors. All of these mutants had been obtained and described by us, and the stock used by Bancroft and Loeb was obtained in part at least from my friend Dr. Frank E. Lutz, who had at that time in his possession, as a letter I have from him states, certainly two of these mutants, black and minia- ture, that he had received from me. It seems to me not improbable that the collector, who got the stock from Dr. 1 Science, XXXIII, 1911. 2 Loe. cit. 710 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vow XLVII Lutz for Professor Loeb, included by mistake some flies heterozygous for these two characters; for in our very extensive experience with wild stock from Cold Spring Harbor (the origin of most of Dr. Lutz’s stock) and else- where these mutants have never arisen again. . At various times experiments have been made in this laboratory involving wide ranges of temperature,® salts, sugars, acids, alkalis without any resulting mutation. In fact, our experience with Drosophila has given us the impression that mutations are rare events, although the actual number of our mutants is now quite large. Guyénot* also has treated ampelophila to high tempera- tures, to radium and to X-rays without result. When the adult flies were treated with ultra-violet light, however, a definite type of ‘‘black’’ fly was obtained. The first eggs that such females lay are normal and give rise to normal flies. The eggs laid later fail to hatch, although they ap- pear to begin their development. On the third day amongst the abnormal eggs some were found that gave rise to flies that were apparently normal. It happened that they were not examined again until after the flies of the next generation had appeared (many of them had died). Both among the living and the dead flies there was a considerable percentage of black flies. The black females laid eggs which did not develop, even although normal males were added. Why the black males were not also tested by outcrossing is not apparent. The descrip- tion of the black flies given by Guyénot tallies in some points with our stock of ebony in which the females were at first usually infertile but the males fertile. At first, indeed, we kept the stock by breeding the ebony males to the heterozygous females. These are intermediate in color. In fact, Guyénot seems to have had heterozygous flies but did not, according to his account, obtain any black flies from them. However, if the ultra-violet light is a specific agent for these mutations the experiment can easily be repeated. 3 Science, XXII, 1910. 4 Bull. Scientifique, XLVIII, 1914. bg No. 576] MUTATIONS IN DROSOPHILA TII It should be added that only one of Guyénot’s two lines gave dark flies after treatment with ultra-violet light. This might seem to indicate that the first result was acci- dental, or due to the presence of a recessive mutation in the stock prior to treatment were it not that a careful control is recorded. Guyénot himself speaks with much caution concerning the interpretation of his results. De- cision as to their value may be reserved until repetition of the experiment gives confirmation. Our own experience with Drosophila shows that mutations appear under con- ditions where all the other flies in the same culture are normal and we have become unduly sceptical perhaps towards evidence which refers a particular mutant to some unusual treatment to which the flies have been subjected. Until we can get definite information as to how mutants arise, whether through external influences, through acci- dents of mitosis, through hybridizing, or through changes in the chromosomes with its consequent dislocations of the machinery of crossing over, or in some other way, it seems futile to discuss the question. THE ANALYSIS OF A CASE OF CONTINUOUS VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA BY A STUDY F ITS LINKAGE RELATIONS PROFESSOR JOHN S. DEXTER, OLIVET COLLEGE I. Introduction. II. The Germinal Constitution of Beaded Flies. A. Crosses between Beaded and Normal Wild Flies. 1. Behavior in First Generation. 2. Behavior in Second Generation. 3. Behavior in Third and Fourth Generations. B. paa between Beaded Flies and Other Mutants. e F, Generation. $ Linkage Relations. (a) Sex Linkage. _ (b) Linkage to Sex-linked Genes (c) Linkage to Second Chitmoscme Genes. (d) Linkage to Third Chromosome Genes. III. The Effect of Environmental Conditions on the Development of Beaded Wings. A. General Statement. B. The Effects of Relative Moisture C. The Effects of Covering with Paraffine the Mouth of the Bottle in which the Flies are Developing. D. The Effects of Acidity and Alkalinity. E. The Effect of cape ger aie ald F. The Effect of Dar IV. The Effect of Selection on me Production of Different Types of Bead- edness, V. Mutation in Beaded Stock. A, General Statement. B. Perfect Notched Wings. C. Spread Wings D. Stumpy Wings. VI. ee I. INTRODUCTION Hise had the principles of Mendelism been worked out in one species of plant than apparent exceptions to these principles were discovered. Mendel’s own case of the breeding true of species hybrids in Hieracium was the first of these, and since 1900 others have been reported. te 712 No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA 713 Further analysis has shown that many of these early cases are readily interpreted on Mendelian principles, while for other exceptions, like that of Hieracium, for instance, the true explanation has been found without in any way coming into conflict with Mendelism. The masking of a Mendelian ratio may be effected in many ways, and some of the most important of the recent work in genetics has dealt with this problem. Among the conditions so far brought to light may be mentioned the following: (a) Multiple Factors.—Recent papers by MacDowell (1914) and Shull (1914) have discussed at length the literature and history of this subject. In brief, the work that has been done shows that in both animals and plants the production of certain characters is brought about through the action of two or more independently Men- delizing pairs of genes that have similar effects on the developing organism. If the effect of these genes is cumulative, so that the character is more or less produced according to the number of dominant genes present, the ` type of inheritance known as blended inheritance is produced. If the effect is not cumulative, the recessive character does not appear with the frequency of 1:3, but with the frequency of 1:15, 1:63, etc., according to the number of pairs of genes concerned. (b) The Effect of the Environment.—A typical case of this sort is reported by Baur (1912). In crossing a dark red to a red strain of Antirrhinum, a complete series between the red and the dark red appeared in the F, gen- eration; the effect of light on the plants was such that plants that had developed in a bright light had a darker color than those that had developed in a less intense light. The analysis of the F, generation, however, proved con- clusively that one fourth of the F, plants had been homo- zygous dark reds, one fourth had been homozygous red, and two fourths had been heterozygotes. Morgan (1912a) has described a case in Drosophila in which moisture conditions in the bottle in which the flies are developing determine to a certain extent whether or not certain 714 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVII characters shall appear; and Hoge (1914) has shown that certain temperatures are necessary for the develop- ment of reduplicated legs in Drosophila. Other examples may be found in the literature of genetics. (c) Lethal Characters—There have been reported sev- eral instances in recent years of animals and plants which are unable to live if homozygous for certain genes. The No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA 715 case of yellow mice, Baur’s Aurea-strain of Antirrhinum (Baur, 1912) and the modified sex-ratios in Drosophila reported by Morgan (1912d) are examples of this phe- nomenon. The object of the present paper is to describe a case of inheritance in Drosophila that for some years seemed to 716 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII defy Mendelian analysis. Though all the details of the case have not been worked out, enough has been done to show that it is brought about by factors which segregate in the ordinary Mendelian fashion, and that the diffi- culties which it still presents are not opposed to that hypothesis. The case under consideration is that of Beaded wings, which, according to Morgan (1911a), first appeared in May, 1910, among flies that had been exposed during part of their early life to radium rays. : The appearance of these wings can best be understood from the figures (Figs. 1-12), which represent a few of the forms that may appear in a stock culture. All grada- tions may be found between wings perfectly normal and mere strips, such as shown by Figure 11. In the early days of its history, according to Morgan, the Beaded-winged flies did not breed true, but for many generations produced many normal-winged offspring. At the time when I took up the experiment, however, the stock bred almost 100 per cent. pure; that is, almost every fly hatched had wings more or less Beaded. I have at present a strain which breeds true, throwing only Beaded- winged offspring, and most of the offspring have the Beading in an extreme form. Most of my work has been done with this stock. II. THE GERMINAL CONSTITUTION OF BEADED FLIES A. CROSSES BETWEEN BEADED AND WILD FLIES 1. Behavior in First Generation When a Beaded fly is mated to a normal fly of a normal Wild stock, a considerable number of flies with Beaded wings usually appears in the first generation (F',). The percentage is not constant, but varies between zero and about fifty per cent. (See Table I.) From Chart 1, it appears possible that the average percentage of Beaded- = winged offspring per pair is near 10-15 per cent. or else near 30-35 per cent. of the total offspring. The exact No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA Til average is 25.5 per cent. That there is a bimodal curve produced may perhaps not be significant, as will appear TABLE I CROSSES OF BEADED TO WILD FLIES, SHOWING PERCENTAGES OF BEADED- WINGED OFFSPRING TEA EBT 2 ee rea > n a Per Cent. of Flies with Beaded Wings | | Le olaia i a] o a sen $| Si 212] 8] 8] 4/8) sie) 8 (3 tatala sla sida g Number of broods giving this per-| | pe | a E E P EEAS | H 5 10| 6 7 7 14 3a 2 2) 2 Average size of brood......... +--+ ++, 203 172 148 120 180 113 101 95 130 50 73 from the following facts, although later evidence will show that it very possibly is significant. The per cent. of Beaded-winged offspring given by one pair (Beaded X Wild) may vary at different times and ee SER SS eS Aa Se a &£ SE 3s 3 4 aa ae 2 wu 2 asrazse ts R CHART I Numbers of broods giving certain percentages of Beaded-winged off- spring in F, generation of Beaded X Wild. (See Table I). under different conditions. For instance, if a pair are put into a bottle with food and are left there for ten days, and are then put into another bottle with fresh food and left another ten days, the percentage of Beaded-winged offspring will be different in the two broods. Table II gives the records of such tests. The first two were made 718 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII with single pairs. In the third case, a Beaded male was given four virgin females, so that although all the off- TABLE II DIFFERENT PERCENTAGES OF BEADED-WINGED OFFSPRING BY THE SAME PARENTS DURING Two SEPARATE TEN-DAY PERIODS IN DIFFERENT BOTTLES First Ten Days | Second Ten Days | No. of Off- Per Cent. No. of Off- Per Cent. | spring Beaded | spring Beaded First wate 65 00S C196 te a Second pairs. 2.. ks Ss 117 rå | 146 : 22 One father X4 mothers. . 389 20 301 28 spring have the same father, they come from four mothers. Inspection of this table shows that it is quite impossible to assign the parents of any one brood to any definite class based on the percentage of Beaded-winged offspring that they give. Table II shows also that the parents gave a larger per- centage of Beaded-winged offspring during the second ten days than during the first period. That this is a coincidence appears from Table III. Here it is shown from the records of fifty broods chosen at random, that TABLE III PERCENTAGES OF BEADED-WINGED FLIES IN THE First CounT or A BROOD COMPARED WITH THOSE OF THE LAST COUNT (INTERVAL OF FROM EIGHT TO TEN Days). BASED ON COUNTS FROM Firty Broops, CHOSEN AT RANDOM iret | rase | Fit | Læt | rie | raot f Ph | bai | rat | bai Count | Count | Count | Count Count Cvunt Count Count Count | Count 36 zi 10 0 10 33 10 13 3 7 -+ 0 42 24 1 0 25 24 51 30 10 0 71 25 8 1 24 0 23 11 20 0 0 1 19 7 11 3 15 12 1 0 5 5 3 0 32 0 43 8 48 20 64 15 36 10 40 0 42 0 45 4 37 3 25 3 46 9 32 6 37 40 17 6 18 0 60 0 50 40 33 15 10 0 47 10 16 0 29 4 21 0 10 0 28 20 52 0 56 29 Larger percentage of Beaded-winged offspring the first count, 44 broods. Larger percentage of Beaded-winged offspring the last count, 5 broods. ood. : Ee percentage of Beaded-winged offspring both counts, 1 br No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA 719 the counts made in the first few days after the flies of any brood begin to hatch show almost invariably a very much larger percentage of Beaded-winged offspring than do the last counts made. This fact will be considered at some length in the section on environmental effects. Enough has been said, at least, to show that, whether the results here described are genetic or environmental effects, the F, generation is remarkably inconstant with reference to the percentage of Beaded-winged offspring that appear. It is evident that this percentage can be readily altered by (1) changing the length of the period CHART 2. Bd of X Wild? daughters, 33% Bd sons, ji bots RAR Bd o&' X Wild 9 daughters, 16% Bd sons, 3% Bd Bd o& X Wild 2 daughters, 23% Bd sons, 9% Bd during which the brood is allowed to run; (2) by chang- ing the parents from one bottle to another. Extensive studies of environmental effects have shown other ways in which the percentages can be altered, but of this we will treat later. 2. Behavior in the Second Generation The question at once arises whether the Beaded and normal F, flies are alike genetically. To the solution of this problem two different breeding tests were applied: viz., matings of F, normal by normal, normal by Beaded, and Beaded by Beaded; and back crosses of both normal and Beaded to Wild stock. The results of these tests are given in Tables IV and V. These tables show that when — 720 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII Beaded-winged flies of the F, generation are used as par- ents, more Beaded-winged young are produced than when normal-winged F, flies are used. This holds true for each TABLE IV MATINGS BETWEEN F, FLIES OF THE CROSS BEADED BY WILD, SHOWING PERCENTAGES OF BEADED OFFSPRING IN INDIVIDUAL BROODS Per Cent. of Flies Beaded | SPT alalalala ajaja ETE TIT ARTIR TTS lel" leleialals|siei/s|sieis|s Normal XNormal........ }8|2)2]...)... aR es ep iy a Be ty ese Heres i Ut Orme X Denes. rn F T heh Sid S E e a Beaded XBeaded........ Rhee a as ee ee oe i 2 ila TABLE V BACK-CROSSES TO WILD OF F, FLIES OF THE Cross BEADED X WILD, SHOW- ING PERCENTAGES OF BEADED OFFSPRING IN INDIVIDUAL BROODS Percentage of Offspring Beaded alelaleisieal3iel3| erais g s shel Ss] 3I r ii Normal XWild.... o.oo... ZETTE T EA ee le Headed x Wid 03. a a Ba E a a ee ge Q a of the five crosses shown in the two tables. Normal- winged F, flies do, however, have some Beaded-winged off- spring, both whi mated among themselves, and also, though more rarely, when back crossed to Wild. These F, and back-cross results give little satisfaction at first sight to the student of Mendelism. If we suppose that there is one gene on which the Beaded condition depends, and that it is partially dominant, then Beaded TABLE VI BEADED AND NORMAL OFFSPRING BY SEXES WHEN ONE PARENT IS BEADED ND THE OTHER WILD 29 9:9 Pg TF 9 9 Bd. 1,246 s 4,488 948 — 4,481 21.7 | 175 894 | Beaded | Normal Beaded Normal Per Cent. ey se Mother Beaded. 2,959 1,139 2,684 23.2 29.8 No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA 721 and normal F, flies should give the same results when used as parents. Or if we were dealing here with a case like the ‘‘yellow mouse’’ case, in which homozygous yellows do not exist: that is, if homozygous ‘‘Beadeds’’ do not exist, then one quarter of the flies produced by two Beaded parents from the stock should be normal. But as was said before, the stock breeds true, every fly produced having Beaded wings. It may be noted that a pair of F, normal flies usually produce less than 10 per cent. of Beaded offspring. If these normal flies carried a recessive gene for Beaded- ness, they should produce twenty-five per cent. Beaded offspring. The.Beaded F, offspring, on the other hand, though they produced in all cases more than twenty-five per cent., did not produce 75 per cent. Beaded offspring, as they should have done if a single. dominant gene for Beaded wings were heterozygous in them. 3. Behavior in Third and Fourth Generations Beaded offspring, that appeared in the F, generation of the cross Beaded X Wild, were back crossed to Wild. The process was again repeated with the Beaded off- spring that appeared, till four generations had been pro- duced. The results of this test are given in Tables VIT and VIII and in Chart 4. A striking result is that an F, Beaded fly or even a fly of later generations heterozygous for Beaded wings some- TABLE VII REPEATED BACK-CROSSES OF BEADED-WINGED FLIES FROM THE Cross BEADED By WILD TO WILD STOCK TO SHOW PERCENTAGES OF BEADED- WINGED OFFSPRING. See Chart IV Family 1| Family 2 | Family 3 |Family4| Family 5| Total ao o i ao o a èo E 1 SE) e R a SE pa | SE |ua | S8 |å. |58 gals E| na. | se | va es S S 5 S oa eration 1......... 86 25.6] 460 28.9] 690) 23.2] 48| 4.2] 82| 15.9]1,266 Generation 2. 226 25.7|1,711| 19.3] 646| 15.9]137 1.5|314| 7.6/3,034) 17 Generation 3. 515 20.8|2,512) 24.6|2,241) 19. 1) 1.8319 16.3]6,038) 21.9 Generation 4......... 135! 8.9] 196| 24.0}..... Te 4.0|132| 25.0| 760| 13.7 722 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII TABLE VIII NoRMAL FEMALES FROM FAMILY 2, GENERATION 2, BACK-CROSSED TO WILD MALES, SHOWING PERCENTAGES OF BEADED-WINGED OFFSPRING ¢ Ba. | Beaded | Normal 2 | 1,040 | .02 Sas Doo: times has as large a percentage of Beaded-winged off- spring when mated to Wild, as does a fly direct from pure Beaded stock when mated to Wild, though a comparison of Tables I and V shows that this is not the usual occur- rence. This suggests at once the action of a lethal gene (Morgan, 1912b). Morgan has shown that in a certain stock of Drosophila there are twice as many females as males in the offspring of one half the females. No matter - to what male such a female be mated, her daughters are twice as numerous as her sons, and one half of her daughters also repeat this phenomenon, and one half of the daughters of these again. This fact finds its explana- tion in the assumption that there is in one of the sex- chromosomes of such females a gene which prevents the development of any male which gets it. Now if such a gene had the power of expressing itself as a dominant in those flies that carried it in the hetero- zygous condition, if, for example, it caused the wings to be Beaded, it would be possible to select such flies at sight, and these flies could then be depended upon to repeat the phenomenon. (Morgan accomplishes the same end by mating such flies to mutants carrying a gene with which the lethal gene shows close linkage, such as that for white- eyes. He then finds that the red-eyed females carry the lethal gene, unless, as rarely happens, a ‘‘eross-over’’ has occurred.) Such a sex-linked lethal gene producing a dominant wing character has actually been found to occur in the case of a mutant which arose in the Beaded stock, and which will be discussed later. For the present we must note that if the lethal gene were not associated with sex, - its presence could be detected by the absence of certain No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA 3 723 expected ratios, or classes, or in some other peculiarity of genetic behavior. In the case before us, we found that the F, generation consisted of at least two types; viz., Beaded and not-Beaded flies. These were shown to differ genetically. To obtain such a result must mean that at least one of the parents was heterozygous in at least one gene. This result is however a fairly constant one; and by virtue of the long-continued inbreeding of the Beaded stock this heterozygosity must surely have been weeded out before now if there were no serious hindrance to homozygosity. The classic example of this sort of effect is that of the yellow mice. But the development of Beaded wings can not be brought about by the action of a single lethal gene, for if this were true it would be impossible to obtain a stock of Beaded flies that would breed true, and yet such a stock, as has already been said, is the one from which these very crosses derive their Beaded ancestors. There must therefore be at least one pair of allelomorphs of which one member is effective in producing Beaded wings, and can exist in the homozygous condition and possibly also another pair of allelomorphs of which one member is a recessive lethal gene. We can explain many of the facts so far obtained on the supposition, that there are these two independently Mendelizing pairs of allelo- morphs concerned in the production of Beaded wings. The pair containing the lethal gene we will call L (nor- mal) and | (lethal); and the other pair B’ (Beaded) and b’ (normal). The occurrence of the two genes B’ and 1 in one individual usually causes such an individual to have Beaded wings, though Beaded-winged flies also occur which do not carry the lethal gene, but are homo- zygous for B’. | It should be possible then to isolate a stock of Beaded- winged flies not carrying this lethal factor, 1. Such flies should give a much smaller percentage of Beaded-winged offspring in the F, generation of a cross with Wild stock (or perhaps none at all, if B’ were recessive), than would those flies carrying IL. Such a stock has not yet been ob- 724 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vor. XLVIII tained, but occasionally a strain of Beaded flies is met with that gives only low percentages of Beaded-winged offspring. See, for instance, Family 4, Table VII. Pos- sibly such a stock would not be recognized at once, espe- cially if it were so affected by environmental conditions that even flies homozygous for the factor B’B’ sometimes had normal wings. Normal-winged flies, as will be pointed out in a later section of this paper, do very fre- quently appear in Beaded stock, but these flies when mated to each other appear to throw as many Beaded- winged offspring as do the Beaded-winged flies of the stock, and often 100 per cent. of their offspring have Beaded wings. In this connection it will be of interest to recall that Chart 1, and Table I gave results that might be inter- preted as evidence of the bimodal curve that should be expected if the above hypothesis is correct. Normal females from the second generation of Family 2 were also back-crossed to Wild males. The results are given in Table VIII. Most of these normal females gave very few or no Beaded offspring (Type X) while two of them gave a considerable number of Beaded offspring (Type Y). The explanation here is perhaps that the type Y females were genetically like most of the Beaded females of an F, generation (on our hypothesis, B’ L b’ 1) while the females of Type X were genetically lacking in the factors that are usually present in Beaded F, flies (i. e., they were B’ L b L). That such an occurrence is not infrequent in Drosophila is seen in Table IV in which three broods out of fifteen raised from normal F, flies gave 25 per cent. or more of Beaded offspring though the other twelve broods gave less than fifteen per cent., and eight broods less than five per cent. of Beaded offspring. It seems certain therefore that there are two types of normal-winged offspring in the F, generation of the cross, Beaded by Wild; one of these is genetically like the Beaded flies of the same generation and the other is genetically different from its Beaded brothers and sisters. Types X and Y have been found to occur in all of the No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA 725 tests made of F, flies whether of matings to Wild stock or of matings to other mutants such as Black, Pink, Are, Ebony, ete. Table XXVI shows these two types as they appeared in back crosses to normal Pink males of normal and Beaded females of the cross Pink Beaded by Wild. Here it was found that more of the normal than of the ‘Beaded F, flies were of Type X, and conversely that more of the Beaded than of the normals were of Type Y. It has not been possible to distinguish with certainty between these two types even by their offspring because of the large amount of fluctuation that occurs in the per- centages of Beaded offspring. For example it would be difficult to say whether a fly giving five per cent. of its offspring Beaded would belong to Type X or Type Y. It would be expected that Type Y would be given by those flies that carried both factors for Beaded, and Type X by those that lack the lethal factor. and it wil) be seen later that on the whole the evidence supports this view. B. Crosses BETWEEN BEADED FLIES AND OTHER MUTANTS 1. The F, Generation If we examine the F, generation when Beaded flies are crossed to other mutants, i. e., to flies of a stock that is perfectly normal so far as Boadedness is concerned, but which is unlike the normal Wild flies in some other wing character, or in eye color or body color, etc., we find an even greater amount of variability in the percentage of Beaded-winged offspring than in the F, generation of Beaded by Wild. (See Tables I and IX; also Charts 1 and 3.) The details may be gathered from Table IX, where it can be seen that there is a certain specificity in the per- centage of Beaded offspring that appear in any specific mating. For instance, it appears that more of the offspring have Beaded wings if a cross is made with Vermilion- eyed flies than when Beadeds are mated to Pink-eyed 726 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII TABLE IX THE PERCENTAGES OF BEADED-WINGED FLIES IN THE F, GENERATION OF ROSSES BETWEEN BEADED FLIES AND OTHER MUTANTS Percentages EEPE aidiaie gdigiy Ls Jeon Rabari Se o eer a ae Vermillion: 21). ie BET Bard Ae | EAE ee eel. Pe ie Miniature Eosin... 1 EEN E noe e aaa. Pink Be aded X White i124 JL] Apak a] Pink Beaded XWild.....)4/4/4/1/1]1)..)....)..).. ME Sea Behe tec Lege E Pink Ebony.. 2: 222: ape Lj fee| Dl. feels BY rod Sue ape cg 1 UP a a ok Da ma Des ee fee AE leche baa eerie ns iil Pea ack E a e a k IER RA AN ears PMN. ia et eee pape |* chased e Ble aime Ey cep be aero Voie 05 1 ee pibes toate ts als | a Liste e el ate eas nai eer Spt ego Se Bet ad ok PoP) Bae Sua A Pee an Gry Gr om Figo Os aed Bg GPE es Eh a Bap RRO a i ee a a a a ees Res ard cee oo SENDA Gap 8/6/5)1/5/6)8/5/7/2)-./2/1]..[ hfe ladat 1 fas Beaded X Wild (Table I).| 1 |5 [10/6 |7 |7 |14|3/2)2/2]..]..)..]..[..[..]..[.h. Grand Total... 9 |11)15) 7 1213221810 l4 21 1 Eh, fi i S ek CHART III %8 * oa <8 EE YR 2k SB ce wa Sf eS eh PR CRSA Ee 8 eed =x WSS Se 2 2 AS a EE a WRMARBRARRRWIVSRRRa Zr aa Distribution of broods giving certain percentages of Beaded-winged off- sprin Beaded X Normal (other Mutants or Wild). (See Table ı generation of r No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA 121 flies, or more in the crosses with White-eyed flies than in those with Black body color. (In every case, where the contrary is not stated the flies are normal in other respects than the one named, e. g., White-eyed flies in these crosses have Gray bodies and Long normal wings.) No explanation of this specificity by the assumption of a segregation of factors in the germ cells appears to be available here, though such a possibility has not yet been ruled out, or can be ruled out till certain other phe- nomena are understood. The easiest way of ‘‘explain- ing’’ it is that the dominance of the genes for Beadedness varies in accordance with many other circumstances, among which are differences in the other genes present, such as those for Vermilion, White or Pink. Such an assumption as this, as will appear later, would seem to be fully in accord with the behavior of the genes for Beaded wings when in still different relationships. It is assumed, then, for example, that the percentages of Beaded-winged flies in the F, generation of a cross between Beaded and White are higher than those in the F, generation of a cross between Beaded and Black, be- cause the gene for Black is relatively to the gene for White eyes an inhibitor of Beadedness. It would appear as though a series might be made of the mutants of - Drosophila beginning with those in which the genes for Beaded wings are most dominant and ending with those in which the Beaded genes are recessive. In order to construct such a series a large number of pairs would have to be made for each cross in order to determine the limits of variability of Beadedness for the cross econ- cerned. The work would probably be greater than the value of the results obtained, and therefore the attempt has not been made to earry out this test. From what has been done incidentally in carrying out other experiments, it will be seen that in general the darker eye colors and body colors are associated with a low percentage of Beadedness in the F, generation, and the brighter colors with a higher percentage. This may, however, only be a coincidence. 728 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII 2. Linkage Relations (a) Sex Linkage If in the crosses thus far described the sex of parents and offspring that show Beaded wings be considered, it may appear at first as though we may be dealing with a partially sex-linked gene. For it very frequently happens that when the mother is Beaded, and the father is normal (either of Wild stock or of some mutant stock not carry- ing Beadedness), more of the sons than of the daughters are Beaded. For example, in one such brood, there were 17 Beaded to 128 normal females, and 5 Beaded to 130 normal males, or 12 per cent. of the females and 3.5 per cent. of the males. Both of these examples were deliberately chosen because they were good ex- amples of the phenomenon described. It would be possi- ble to select from my records several examples of the reverse phenomenon, where Beaded females had more Beaded daughters than Beaded sons, and where Beaded males had more Beaded sons than Beaded daughters. Nevertheless, the records of all broods available have given the numbers shown in Table VI, where it appears that more sons are Beaded when only the mother is Beaded and more daughters when only the father is Beaded. It may perhaps be significant, on the other hand, that when the mother is Beaded a slightly larger percentage of her daughters is Beaded than of the daughters of a Beaded male, while a very much larger percentage of her sons is Beaded than the sons of a Beaded male. In other words, it seems that the daughters are affected to ap- proximately the same extent, whether they get their Beadedness from father or mother, while the sons are affected also by the mother, whether or not she carries Beadedness. This might mean that there is some gene in the sex chromosome that does not show except when other Beaded factors are present. That this is not the case will appear from Chart 2, which records three generations of flies in each of which the mother was normal (Wild) No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA 729 and the father Beaded. This shows that although the father transmitted his Beadedness more to his daughters than to his sons, yet his Beaded sons also had the capac- ity to affect their daughters more than their sons, and these sons again repeated the phenomenon. Yet these males could not have received their X-chromosome from their father, unless non-disjunction (see Bridges, ’13) had occurred. In fact, to produce the results here given non-disjunction must occur in one half the females of the Wild stock. Frequent tests with the Wild stock by practically all of the students in the laboratory make it certain that this is not the case. I also tested a consider- able number of the females by mating them to sex-linked mutants and found no non-disjunction. This apparent sex-linkage that does not follow the ‘ordinary rules’’ of sex-linkage must be left for the time being as one of the still unsolved problems. The only possibility of explanation that occurs to me is that the above-described effect would be produced if in the cyto- plasm of the egg of the Beaded female something were present which is absent in the egg of the normal female, and to which the males are more responsive in their development than are the females. This suggestion has not a particle of cytological evidence to support it. Mor- gan (1912d) has suggested that the influence of cytoplasm may cause certain peculiar results obtained in crosses between Miniature-winged and Rudimentary-winged flies. (b) Linkage to Sex-linked Genes Matings of Beaded flies to flies with sex-linked char- eaters, including Vermilion and Vermilion-yellow, have been made and the F, generation raised. No sign of linkage was observed. The F, figures are given in Tables X and XI. These eases definitely establish that there is no gene for Beaded wings in the X-chromosome. Although no sex-linked gene for Beaded wings are known, there has arisen in the Beaded stock by mutation a fly with notched wings (Fig. 13) that proved to be 730 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII TABLE X F, COUNTS FROM THE CROSS VERMILION 2 X BEADED ¢ hese Q rg bg Q pioi oe a Q pyel et 9 sarod % Bd o No. of Bd. if | no coupling exists Bd.V : N.V.= 65: oig a E a, e | ree ba Red: NeR 55o Talore ae: 28.0 Bd.Total : N Total = "i20: ‘Hii SOP wine ae 27.8 | (120) TABLE XI F, COUNTS FROM THE CROSS VERMILLION YELLOW X BEADED Pens" Normal} Beaded | Normal | Beaded | Normal | Beaded | Normal Gra Gray Gr Gray Yellow Yellow Yellow | Yellow ed | Vermil. | Vermil. | Red Red | Vermil. | Vermil. Zar 35 | 34 41 34 | aa AT 88 Expected No. of Bd. if % Beaded a coupling exists 44.1 01.7 z Les. 5 N Ro 2068 20l La a e a | Bav NV © S61 oo 41.0 5 | Bd.G. : N. G. = g ‘ >y IEE POT AE SS |. 43.4 199.1 PMS, NY etc Paa 42.7 92.0 | Bd.Total : N. vor gk one We ee ROE 43.2 | (291.0) TES caused by a dominant sex-linked factor lethal when homozygous. (See page 754.) It will be dis- cussed under the name ‘‘Perfect Notched’’’ and its peculiarities described in the section on ‘‘ Mu- tation in Beaded Stock.” (c) Linkage to Second Chromo- some Genes For the reasons given, it seems certain that there is in the group of sex-linked genes no gene con- cerned inthe production of Beaded wings. We shall later bring for- ward evidence to show that there is such a gene in the third chromo- some group. (Sturtevant, 1913.) | The crosses made with flies showing characters whose No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA 731 genes are in the second chromosome are still perplexing; for while the second chromosome exerts an influence on the statistical results, as will be evident from the figures to be presented, the nature of this influence is not fully determined. The second chromosome characters with which tests have been made are the wing characters, Are, Curved, Vestigial, Antlered, and Strap,' the body color, Black; and the eye color, Purple. Vestigial, Antlered, and Strap stand for wing char- acters of such a nature that it is not possible to distin- guish Beaded-winged individuals if any of these other characters are also present. They are therefore of no use for determining whether or not there is a second chromosome gene for Beaded wings. The crosses be- tween Beaded flies and flies with these characters do not especially interest us here. It may be said in passing, however, that in every case in the F, generation between these flies and Beaded flies, from 60 to 90 per cent. of the offspring had non-normal wings, and the author was put to serious straits to classify the new wing types that appeared. These were similar in all the crosses, however, and on the whole resembled Beaded wings. In the F, generation, and in back crosses to Beaded Stock and to Vestigial Stock further complications arose with more new types of wings, including a new ‘‘muta- tion’’ which bred true from the start, and which will be discussed briefly and described under the name ‘‘Spread’’ in the section that concerns mutation. We may now return to the crosses between Beaded flies and Black, or Purple, or Are, or Curved. These crosses give results that can be used for the study of linkage, and they present in common a number of distinguishing 1 Strap Wings is a mutant much resembling Extreme Beaded in appear- ance but its mode of inheritance has not yet been worked out. It may be that it actually is Beaded plus some at present unknown gene. Beadedness is suspected to occur also in Vestigial and Antlered stock. This may very likely be true since Strap and Antlered arose in Vestigial, and Vestigial in Beaded. 732 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII characteristics. Tables XII to XX give the results in sauces form. TABLE XII F, Counts oF THE Cross BEADED 2? X CURVED 3 | Beta Norii Be aded No on | ‘ta Ba. a. | Exp. B | Curved Curved Straight Straight | Curved | Straight Type to o] å | 61 | 16 | 246 | 4 | 16 E Pewee. | 2 25 18 105. | 3.6 16.4 One of the most striking characteristics of these second chromosome crosses is that the F, flies fall into two classes or possibly into three classes with reference to the offspring that they produce. These classes I have called Type 1, Type 2, and Type 3. In Type 1 there is no linkage between Beaded wings and the second chromosome character, but Beaded- winged flies occur with equal frequency in all classes of TABLE XIII F, Counts oF THE Cross BEADED 2? X ARC ĝ Beaded Normal Beaded Normal Exp. Bd. | Exp. Bd. Are Are Straight Straight Are Straight Tyee i... | 39 | 200 | 184 | 970 | 98+ | 185 SYDO De. oe sas 41 152 330 452 73 t BGS offspring. In Type 2 there is linkage of Beaded wings with the second chromosome characters, so that the Beaded wings appear more frequently in flies showing the characters of the Beaded parent. In Type 3, which occurs only a very few times and is not very marked except in Table XX, Beaded wings appear to a greater percentage in the offspring whose other characters are not those of the Beaded parent. (J. ¢.,‘‘repulsion’’ occurs between the factor for Beadedness and that for the second chromosome character with which it entered the cross.) I do not wish to emphasize Type 3, but concern- ing the other two it is important to note that about one half of the F, flies seem to be of Type 1 and one half of | Typo 2, In re 2 ae in the cases here adduced the linkage is No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA 733 strongest with the character Are and weakest with Black. If there is a gene in the second chromosome which aids in producing Beaded wings, it seems probable that it is located nearer to Are than to Black and on the side of Are away from Black. TABLE XIV BACK-CROSSES OF F, BEADED-WINGED MALES OF THE Cross BEADED ¢@ X PURPLE CURVED 2 TO PURPLE CURVED FEMALES OF NORMAL STOCK Normal Pr. Beaded Red Normal Red Exp. Nor. | Exp. Nor. Beaded Pr, Curved Curved | Straight | Straight Bd. Pr. Cv.) Ba. . Strt. Hopsa So 15 125 | 15 | 154 | 13.6-| 164 Types. Gee ee 9 28 OV We io ia It remains to consider Type 1, and to find the reason for the existence in the F, generation of flies whose off- spring show no linkage between Beadedness and second chromosome characters, and in the same brood, flies whose offspring do show such linkage. The most obvious ‘‘ex- planation’’ would be, of course, that the factor in the TABLE XV BACK-CROSSES OF F, BEADED-WINGED FEMALES OF THE Cross BEADED ¢ X PURPLE CURVED 2? TO PURPLE CURVED MALES OF NORMAL STOCK Brood 2 is of Type 3 for Purple and of Type 1 for Curved. -| B, Pr. | N, Pr. Ba, Red| N, R. | Bd. R. | N. R. | | Us : SEER ents eee tas Boots: os od art oe 6 0 B 44} 7 second chromosome was a ‘‘lethal’’ such as the factor 1, described in an earlier section of this paper. If this were the case, there should be some flies in the Beaded stock homozygous for L, the normal allelomorph of this gene, and also for B’, i. e., B'LB’L. These flies should have fewer Beaded offspring than those heterozygous for 1, and none of these F, offspring should give linkage with second chromosome characters. As a matter of fact, in the F, results given in Table XVI for Beaded by Black, no Bbc was observed; but this case is not good evi- dence, for it was made in the first attempts to solve the problem of Beaded wings, and I had not yet learned the — 734 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vow XLVIII value of F, counts, matings in pairs, and back-crosses to normal. It stands however as the only evidence of its sort that I can give at present. TABLE XVI F, COUNTS OF THE Cross BEADED 9 X BLACK ¢ l : Beaded glen | on Normal | Expected 5 ais Black Epo Gray z Bd. Bl. Gar. Tyl 19 eee 1602 | 19 82 Type 3 is not easy to explain. There are no known cases of this sort elsewhere in Drosophila and I prefer not to attempt to answer this question at present. TABLE XVII BACK-CROSSES al F, MALES OF THE Cross BEADED g X BLACK 2 TO BLACK FEMALES OF NORMAL STOCK Beaded Normal Beaded Normal | Expected nde ty Black Black Gray Gray No, Bd. Bl. Gray faan ORS sec Zz 162 9 187 7.3 8.6 EVO 2 pice wee 5 110 40 97 20.5 24.5 In general, it may be noted that technical difficulties have disturbed the crosses with second chromosome char- acters. The wing character Are is not always easy to recognize, as it is very often nearly normal in appearance. On the other hand, the flies with Curved wings, though always distinct, occasionally get ‘‘stuck up’’ with the food and in their bedraggled condition it can not always be determined whether or not the wings are Beaded as well as Curved. I was at first inclined to attribute the apparent coupling (which was discovered for Are and for Curved before it was discovered for Black) to errors made in the counts. As for Black, the F, and later generations give a much lower percentage of Beaded offspring than do most other crosses, and this necessitates raising large numbers of offspring. The results are, pokes: trustworthy when apie’: No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA 735 The crosses with Purple-eyed flies presented no diffi- culties but ran smoothly aside from the fact that the purple-eyed flies had Curved wings, and as remarked above, Curved wings sometimes get bedraggled. TABLE XVIII BACK-CROSSES OF F, FEMALES OF THE Cross BEADED £ X BLACK È ro BLACK MALES OF NORMAL STOCK Beaded No — | Beaded Normal | Expected | Net Ba. Black Blac | Gray Gray |Nọo, Bd. Bl. Typs o. 7 5 Lge og | 91 | 3.3 | 3.7 Type 2 bee ee) a57 k a8 493 16.5. 175 (d) Linkage to Third Chromosome Genes We have said tentatively that there was perhaps a non- sex-linked lethal gene for Beaded wings in the second chromosome, and that possibly the cytoplasm carried by the egg disposes males toward or away from Beadedness according to whether the fly that bore the egg was or was not Beaded. These relations are not securely deter- mined, and the data are still incomplete. The relation of Beaded wings to characters whose genes are in the third chromosome is much clearer. All crosses that bear on this problem point to one fact, namely, that there is a gene for the production of Beaded wings in the third chromosome, and that this gene is very closely linked to Ebony, and very loosely linked to Pink. Tests have been made between Beaded and the third chromosome char- acters, Maroon, Sepia, and Pink eyes and E OnT body color. TABLE XIX F, COUNTS OF THE Cross PINK BEADED gd X BLACK 2 Pink Pink Pink Bd.! Pink N.| Red Bd. | Red N. | Red Bd.| Red N. Bd. Bl. | N. BL Gray Gray Black Black Gray Gray | Type 1 (?) 2 25 20 57 0 45 3 278 | Expected No. Bd. if no. coupling occurs | Bd.Pink : N.Pink = Fant "3 OREO RGA ON nse rene eet 6 | Ba Rod = Ned. = 3:9238.. rer eis 19 Bd.Black : KAA. = T 1 E a A rae ee | Bd.Gray : N.Gray = pa Ver E EE A 21 | T Ay N.Total = 25 : ba festa VS (25) | 736 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII In the cross of Beaded by Maroon-eyed flies, 1,369 flies were raised in the F, generation. Fifty-seven of these flies had Beaded wings; only one of the Beaded-winged flies had Maroon eyes, while fifty-six were red-eyed. (See Table XXI.) TABLE XX. BACK-CROSSES OF F, FEMALES OF THE Cross PINK BEADED ¢@ X BLACK 9 TO PINK BLACK MALES FROM NORMAL STOCK Pink Bd. | Pink ar Pax Bd.! Pink N. e ep Red N. ae Bd.| Red N. Black | Blac Gra Black | Gray | Gray Type tens: 5 98 8 | 113 5 92 1 | 122 aeaa aio cs. 12 68 6 | 58 7 78 3 | 76 Sa d ae ee eee eee del a l Expected No. Beaded if no upling occurs Ba Piik: N Pank St Bele a ps oe vse eee 23 Baked : N Rod = 16 : 30B. oe Saye 24 Bd- Black: N.Biack = 20 2336. ena 23 Bd .Gray : N Oray = 418 2 360) Pcs. i ns a es Bg oul : N TO ae aro OD Oe ee | (47) F, males of the cross Sepia by Beaded were back- erossed to normal Sepia females. Inasmuch as cross- overs probably do not occur in the male (Morgan, 1912c), no Beaded Sepia flies should occur in the offspring of this cross. Table XXII shows that none occurred. The num- bers are not large, but since they are entirely in accord with the other third chromosome results, it was not thought worth while to increase them. That apparent cross-overs may very rarely occur will appear possible when we consider the results of crossing Beaded by Pink, and the probable significance of the phenomenon will be considered. TABLE XXI F, RESULTS OF THE Cross BEADED Ọ X MAROON ¢ Bd. Maroon| N.Maroon | Bd.Red | N,Red | Exp.Bd.N. | Exp. Bd. R. 1 | 318 | 56 | 994 13 44 In F, counts of the crosses involving Beaded and the body color Ebony, totaling 4,417, in which 1,205 Beaded- winged offspring occurred, not one had the body color _ Ebony, and only eleven had Pink eyes. Repeated attempts No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA tor to obtain Ebony flies with Beaded wings have failed. The possibility that for some ‘‘inherent peculiarity’? an Ebony fly can not have Beaded wings has suggested itself, TABLE XXII BACK-CROSSES OF F, BEADED MALES OF THE Cross BEADED g X SEPIA 2 TO SEPIA FEMALES OF NORMAL STOCK Bd. Sepia | N. Sepia | Bd. Red | N. Red 0 | 134 | 9 | 132 and although this would seem very improbable, it may nevertheless be the fact. At any rate, it appears that Beadedness either depends on genes which in the presence of the Ebony body color are completely recessive, or that the third chromosome gene for Beadedness, B’, lies so close to that for Ebony that cross-overs are extremely rare even in the female. TABLE XXIII F, RESULTS OF THE CROSS BEADED 9 X EBONY ¢ | Beaded Ebony | Normal Ebony | Beaded Gray | Normal Gray 0 | wi ae 17 | 525 Very extensive experiments were carried out with Pink- eyed flies. The important facts brought out are presented in condensed form in Tables XIX, XX, and XXIV-XXIX. In Table XXIV are shown the F, counts for Beaded by | Pink Ebony. From the eleven Pink Beaded flies obtained a new stock was derived, which was ‘‘purified’’ by a few TABLE XXIV F, RESULTS OF THE Cross BEADED Ẹ X PINK EBONY ¢ es g Bå. P. Gray LS ee N. P. Eb. 847 “Bd. R. Eb. | N R. Eb. N. P. Gray Ba. R. Gray N. R. Gray | 0 f 1% 167 | L77 | LS% Expected No. Bd. if no coup- | ling occurs g 738 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII CHART IV Family 1. Bd 9, 86; 25.6% Bd 9, 226 ; 25.7% I l Bd Ẹ, 110; 27.3% Bd g’, 405; 19% Bd g’, 135 ; 8.9% Family 2. Bd , 460; 28.9% Båg Bd g Bd Bd oB d d l | | d F € Bdo Bið Bas? Båg Bde 154 248 129 47.4% 106 88.86 14.9% 12.9% 5.4% 135 186 192 161 23.7% 14.7% 81.26 289% ] | |] | l Big Big Bde Bdg Big BAF BAF Ba? BAP BAF BAF Bag Bag Ba 158 177 162 147 22 47 46.8% 38.4% 8.69 143 231 2 8.6% 42.9% 2 ae 15.54 37.66 11.9% 2824 03.54 13.49% 1624 134 Py i4 Bi a Big Bd g 106 27 63 19.8% 33 Bid, 26 k Bd o, 193 Bag, 237 50% 16.6% Bd 3, 190 9.7% T Bdg a Bdg Bi Bdg Ba Bdg Bda Bdg Bdg Ba Ba as Bdg an Bia Bda Big Bdo 168 185 21S. 185 105. 5T 41.5% 36.94 154s 6.3% 134 al. p 17.8% 16.1% 19% 13.5% 18.7% 20.5% 60.6% 87.26 Family 4. Bd 9, 48; 4.2% Ba ð, 137 ; 1.5% Bd A, 209 ; 2.4% ag Bd f, 237; 4.2% Family 5. Bd 9, 82; 15.9% Ba i 814; 7.6% J Ba d, 175; 9.7% Bd 7, 144; 24,3% Bd d, 50; 32% Bd of, 82; 20.7% o back-erosses of Beaded X Wild in successive generations, show- ing sex of — pori number of offspring, and percentage of offspring No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA 739 generations of selection, and now gives approximately 100 per cent. Beaded offspring, though no selection has been practised for nearly a year. This stock has been used in one series of crosses to supplement another series in which Pink and Beaded enter the cross from opposite parents. The results in each case are essentially similar, and show that when Beadedness enters with Red it comes TABLE XXV F, RESULTS OF THE CROSS PINK BEADED X WILD Bd. Pink | wo | 47 Normal Pink | Beaded Red | Normal Red ‘Exp. No. Bd. P.|Exp. No. Bd. R. | } | o ee 213 | 366 | 964 out more with Red than with Pink. They show that in the F, female crossing over occurs almost independently of Pink, so that almost the same percentage of Beaded- winged individuals appears in each class, though usually the class that is similar to the Beaded parent is consider- ably the largest. In Table XXVII, however, a record is given in which a very considerable ‘‘repulsion’’ oc- curred, and the high Beaded class is not Pink Beaded, as is there expected, but Red Beaded. The results from back-crosses of the brothers of these females to Pink normal stock show that no mistake was made in record- ing the cross, which therefore, though somewhat surpris- ing, must stand. TABLE XXVI BackK-CROSSES OF F, FEMALES OF THE Cross PINK BEADED X WILD TO PINK MALES oF NORMAL STOCK | Bd. Pink | N.Pink | Bd.Red | N.Red | "B5: po | BER? << oo bo gl. gas 2 $37 | 25 | 2.5 Tie Y... | 71 332 58 369 | 626 | 66.4 teal. | za | w 60 706 “ae The tables show also that in the males, crossing over is of very rare occurrence, if, indeed, it occurs at all. The records show that out of 566 Beaded flies (Tables XX VII and XXIX) which occurred as the offspring of an F, male 740 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII back-crossed to Pink normal stock, six flies of the cross- over class appear. For reasons to be mentioned, it is improbable that these represent cross-overs, however, but rather they may be due perhaps either to the presence of the second chromosome gene, l, which usually does not manifest itself in the absence of the third chromosome gene, or to mutation, or to some unknown abnormality. Through carelessness only one of these males was tested TABLE XXVII BACK-CROSSES OF F, MALES OF THE CROSS PINK BEADED X WILD TO PINK FEMALES OF NORMAL STOCK heamana. ia T sean a S un se? rote — TAAT? ER AE REA rar UN ne ae See SR GO AEA IY eam aaa on Sena BR ty a Bd. Pink | N. Pink | Bd.Red | N.Red |Exp. No. Bd. P. Exp. No. Bd. R. | | 56 | 710 | 5 | 805 b> ae | 32 or used further in breeding. They were very slightly Beaded, and had only a very slight ‘‘nick’’ at the tip of the wing, even smaller than that shown in Fig. 3. The single Pink Beaded male mentioned in Table XXIX was mated to several females but was sterile. Another test is also possible, and was made as follows. Pink normal males and females from Table XXIX, which of course should not carry the third chromosome gene for Beaded TABLE XXVIII REPEATED BACK-CROSSES OF F, FLIES OF THE Cross PINK BEADED X WILD TO PINK FLIES oF NORMAL Stock Exp. Exp. ae ee i GA Hel MPN ot Type ea 1 228 1 272 1 1 Bred XPINE Oo Type A.o. 24 379 79 383 48 55 ORK. CS ee EA AE E 5k oe os 25 607 80 655 49 56 DOP oss ick ee 7o 193 0| 135 | 35 185. wings, were then mated together, and among their 374 offspring three males with slight ‘‘nicks’’ at the tip of their wings, exactly like those of the Pink Beaded male before mentioned, were produced. One of these males = was sterile. One of the remaining two was fertile, but No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA T41 gave no Beaded offspring either in the first generation or in the F, generation, although nearly one thousand of his grandchildren were carefully examined. The remaining male was abundantly fertile and had one son exactly like his father in appearance (with a slight nick at the tip of the wings). The rest of his offspring were normal. This son was sterile. TABLE XXIX BACK-CROSSES OF F, FLIES OF THE CROSS BEADED X PINK TO PINK FLIES OF NORMAL STOCK Beaded | Normal | Beaded Normal Exp. Bd. | Exp. No. Pink | Pink | Red Red | Pink Beaded R. Fic! XPink 2 .. 1 | 580 | 223 | 282 F192 XPink:ot i: 70 114 | 114 106 | 84 | 100 The results of these tests with five of these supposed ‘‘cross-over’’ males show clearly that they were not nor- mal Beaded flies. As said, they might represent muta- tions, or the dominance of the gene 1, or some abnormality. These are mere guesses, but since there are no authentic cases on record in Drosophila of crossing over in the male sex in those cases where the mutants dealt with are well known genetically, i. e., since the only apparent cases occur in the Beaded wings and some of the other not well-known and peculiar mutants of Drosophila, we are not justified in assuming that such crossing over takes place here. III. THE EFFECT OF ENVIRONMENTAL CONDITIONS UPON THE PRODUCTION OF BEADED WINGS A. QENERAL STATEMENT If we have so far interpreted the evidence correctly we may formulate the following statement as a provisional hypothesis. A gene B’ located in the third chromosome near that for Ebony is directly responsible for the pro- duction of Beaded wings. By itself in the homozygous condition, the fly bearing it may have normal wings, though it usually will have wings somewhat Beaded. In the heterozygous condition, it is rarely, though sometimes, 742 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vor. XLVIII dominant. The conditions so far presented which cause it to be dominant are two. (1) The presence of a gene | in the second chromosome which can not exist in the homozygous condition. (2) The influence, particularly . noticeable in the males, of non-chromosomal constituents of the egg from which the individual arose, so that if the mother had been Beaded, the appearance of Beaded wings in her sons would be increased, and if the mother had been normal the appearance of Beaded wings in her sons would be reduced. Certain facts already brought out (namely, those pre- sented in Tables II and IIT) show that the tale is not yet told. Our hypothesis does not explain the fact that from definite numbers of eggs laid at different periods in the life of an individual very different percentages of Beaded- winged offspring arise, and these differences do not form a definite series progressing to or from a high percentage as the individual grows older, but are extremely irregular. We have not gained control over this phenomenon, but the evidence we have to present points strongly to the suggestion that the environmental conditions are the final determiners of the percentage of the Beaded-winged off- spring. This environmental control might lie in three distinct methods: (1) The destruction of a certain class of offspring by their differential viability. (2) In the case of Table III the results might be explained on the theory that Beaded flies had a shorter life cycle. This supposi- tion has, however, been disproved as follows. Five non- virgin females from Beaded-winged stock and five non- virgin females from normal-winged stock were put to- gether without males in the same bottle. When the off- spring began to hatch they were examined daily. During the first three days 73 flies hatched, of which 11, or 15 per cent., had Beaded wings. During the following five days 261 flies hatched, of which 54, or 20 per cent., had Beaded wings. Since I was particular to take Beaded flies several days old as the parents of these Beaded offspring, the experiment shows that if there is any difference in the length of the larval life, that of normal-winged flies is No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA 743 the shorter. (3) The determination of whether or not a fly of a given germinal constitution shall have Beaded wings. The first of these effects is probably not the significant one, in view of the following facts. Although as a rule F, normal flies give few Beaded off- spring, and F, Beaded-winged flies relatively many, nevertheless, as has been said, at times normal flies give a high percentage of Beaded offspring and, occasionally, Beaded flies give a low percentage. This can only mean that the dominance of the factor B’ is variable, and con- sidering the large number of times that it shows itself as a recessive, it must be that this varying dominance has a marked effect on the percentage of Beaded-winged off- spring that appear. The possible amount of variation in the environment surrounding a brood of Drosophila developing under laboratory conditions is enormous, even when the attempt is made to keep conditions constant. These variations depend upon the exact ripeness of the bananas used as food, the length of time the food has been fermenting, the amount of food and filter paper used, the size of the bottle in which the larve are developing, the tightness of the cotton plug, the temperature of the laboratory, ete. Due to these causes there arise very great differences in the relative moisture content and carbon dioxide content. If the food is not properly prepared it may rot instead of fermenting, or it may mould, or the reaction may be in one bottle quite alkaline and in another very acid. A perfect control thus becomes an impossibility, and there- fore the experiments to be described must be considered as trials only, and not as decisive tests. In all the experiments on this subject, Beaded flies of pure stock were mated to normal flies of Wild stock in order to learn the effect of particular environments on the percentage of Beaded offspring in the F, generation. On our hypothesis, the pure Beaded flies from stock should be of two kinds, viz., those with the lethal gene 1 (i. e., B’IB’L), and those without 1 (i. e., B’LB’L). Correspond- ingly there should be two types of offspring in the F, 744 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou XLVIII generation, one of which (B’lb’L) should have a consider- ably higher percentage of Beaded offspring than the other (B’Lb’L). If it is possible, however, that B’ should be dominant in the heterozygous condition and in the absence of 1, then it should also be possible theoretically to produce an F, generation every individual of which should have Beaded wings, while those with | as well as (constituting one half the progeny) should have a more extreme form of Beading. In practice it is not usual even under the best of conditions to get more than 40 per cent. of Beaded-winged flies, while, as has been seen, the average amount is about 25 per cent. B. Tar Errect or RELATIVE MOISTURE Table XXX and Charts 5 and 6 present the data for TABLE XXX PERCENTAGES OF BEADED-WINGED FLIES IN THE F, GENERATION IN RELA- 7 Y WET AND Dry Borties. Nor DONE IN PAIRS, BUT EACH BOTTLE CONTAINED SEVERAL PAIRS Dry Bottles Wet Bottles No.1 No. 2 No, 3 No. 4 No.5 No, No. No. No, No. Flies | #84.) Flies 4% Bd. | Flies | #Bd.| Flies | ¥BA-| Flies | # Ba. ist count.: 27%: LG 82:1 48:7 | 26 26.9 | 14-286) 32 2d count. <. ss-a 122 | 14.7 68 | 17.2 79 | 32.9 BT | 20.7 (IB 207 Sd pount, e a 34 | 20.6 19 | 21.1 24 | 41.6 16 | 37.5 37 | 37.9 4th ecunt.<.. 3. oo N 34 8.8 17 | 41 43 | 41.7 Sth oount. cs 59 | 20.81 72 250i 35 |314] 30 | 86.7 | 53 189 6th count. ..:.:. 95 | 10.5 37 | 30.1 LORE oc ei oe 281 | 17.8 | 252 | 24.6 | 203 | 33.5 | 164 | 28 290 29.3 Total Dr... 533 % Bd. 20.5 Total Wet, 657 | % Bd. 30.3 Counts not made every day. Bottle No. 2 was very dry and the flies very small during time of last two counts. this test. The parents were put into bottles of similar size with plenty of food. In three of these bottles the food was very wet and from time to time juice was added in sufficient amount to keep the food saturated. The other two bottles were made relatively dry by putting a No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA 745 CHART V % “I S = > 2 o 2 2 5 5 2 $ - Ea : : = 50%. ee = as “ag 40 h 30% Wet 10% 0% Effect of Relative Moisture in Food on Percentages of Beaded-winged Flies in F, Generation of Beaded X Wild. large amount of filter paper into the bottle at night and removing it the following morning. After two or three - days of this treatment the bottles were so dry that I did not venture to carry the process farther; the flies from CHART VI a a a Š % 3 D 37 > 2 p o E g £ £ £ a4 | * 3 S 3 5, w% y m 20% ean Wet 10% Dry! oF Effect of Relative Moisture in Food on Production of Beaded Wings, as shown by Individual Bottles. 746 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vow XLVIII these dry bottles were rather small and in bottle No. 2, they were extremely small in the last two counts. From Chart 6, where the records are given of the indi- vidual bottles, it will be seen that there is a good deal of irregularity from day to day. Special attention should be called to the curve of pro- duction of bottle 2, which beginning with a high per- centage of Beaded Sifepeine gives fewer and fewer for the first four counts (about six days) and then the per- centage rapidly mounts again. The offspring given dur- ing the last two counts were of surprising minuteness and gave as high a percentage of Beaded individuals as the average of all the bottles on the first day. It has been sug- gested that it may not be wetness or dryness or any one specific thing that brings out the Beadedness, but condi- tions that are unfavorable to the organism as a whole, resulting in poor nourishment. It has frequently been TABLE XXXI THE INFLUENCE OF ACID, ALKALINE, AND FRESH Foop ON THE DEVELOPMENT OF BEADED WINGS Oo |] & we} wi) os D ne . a diale y Eka 2a ERSS Food So Mother Beaded; Father normal........ 26 40 151 129 14.7 23.8 19.1 Father Beaded; Mother normal........ 9 7 | 60 61 13.0 10.3 11.7 ood Fresh Mother Beaded; Father normal........|23| 25; 85 92 21.3 21.4 21.3 Father Beaded; Mother normal........ 54 | 15/147) 164 26.9 8.4 18.2 Food Alkaline Mother Beaded; Father normal ........ 3641| 40; 41 | 46.8 | 50.0 | 48.7 Father Beaded; Mother normal ....... 28 | 16 | 57 58 32.9 21.6 27.7 noted that those bottles which gave very tiny flies gave also a higher percentage of Beaded individuals than the bottles whose flies were of average size. On the other hand, the first flies of a brood are almost invariably larger than the later ones, and yet, as has been seen, they are more Beaded. This is a paradox, but the behavior of bottle No. 2 suggests that as a hatch proceeds and the bottle becomes drier, there may be a certain optimum ~ point for the production of normal winged offspring, and No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA 747 that this point is so low that the flies are poorly nourished for lack of water, though they can survive an even greater water reduction. It is, perhaps, needless to say that an effort has been made after these experiments to keep the moisture con- tent high and fairly uniform in cases where other envi- ronments were being tested. C. THE EFFECTS or COVERING WITH PARAFFINE THE MOUTH OF THE BOTTLE IN WHICH THE FLIES ARE DEVELOPING On observing that the proportion of Beaded to Normal offspring was lowered as a hatch continued, it seemed possible that this might be due to one or to both of two causes: (1) The diminishing water content. This matter has already been considered. (2) To a changing carbon- dioxide content. When a brood is first counted the cotton plug that has been for several days in the mouth of the bottle is removed, and in removing the flies the air within the bottle is very apt to bè much changed. With this pos- sibility in mind a number of bottles were supplied with food and flies, and after ten days (when the larve were beginning to pupate) the parent flies were removed, a little new food put into the bottle and a paraffine cap melted over the cotton so that the bottles were tightly TABLE XXXII Counts or SEVEN BROODS WHICH HATCHED DURING Two PERIODS, THE First oF WHICH WAS SPENT IN A BOTTLE SEALED WITH PARAFFINE, AND THE SECOND IN A BOTTLE COVERED WITH CHEESE CLOTH. BOTTLE No. 7 WAS NOT SEALED WITH PARAFFINE BUT HAD BEEN LIGHTLY STOPPERED WITH COTTON | Bottle 1} Bottle 2 2 Bottles 3 Bottle4 Bottle5 Bottle 6 Bottle 7 sd gliosis cal g 3 33| 3168] g 58 n 4 ZE Se) Mel a ZE] ZE] a E| a ZE S S go ea |75 |36 82/40 54/44 51/45 59/29 18|39 87 | 28 seescvsstvcess.[64| 032] 3/17] 6/22/18! 36) 0/15/13| 52) 2 Total, first count, 426, per cent. Bd., 36. Total, second count, 228, per cent. Bd., 6. Total, both counts, 654, per cent. Bd., 25. 748 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVII sealed. At the same time other bottles were very loosely covered with a light cotton plug. The bottles remained covered till flies had been hatching for four or five days and then the plugs were removed and the flies counted. The paraffine plugs were not replaced; after carefully renewing the air in the bottles, they were covered with cheese cloth and their brood counted again in four days. The results of this test are given in Table XXXII. The results are striking enough at first sight, but I do not know just what their significance is. They show exactly the same phenomenon that is described earlier and illus- trated in Table III. They are more striking than any case I have yet found of the sort, and yet the first infer- — ence drawn, viz., that the markedly higher percentage of Beaded flies in the first count is due to these flies having undergone their late development in a ‘‘close’’ atmos- phere, must be qualified by the statement that ‘‘close’’ does not refer to the carbon-dioxide content. At first suspecting this to be the case, I made an appa- ratus by means of which fresh air could be drawn through a bottle during the entire development of the brood. By this means the carbon-dioxide content could not become very high. In order to prevent drying out, a large amount of food was put into the bottle and the air which was to enter the bottle was first passed through water. The hatching period was prolonged in the cool sink. The re- sults were decisive. One hundred and sixty-nine flies were hatched in the first four days, of which 32 per cent. were Beaded. One hundred and eighty-four flies were hatched in the next four days, of which 10 per cent. were Beaded. The same flies that were the parents of this brood were in the meanwhile transferred to another bottle, which was covered with paraffine. The first four days of hatching gave 108 flies, of which 15 per cent. were Beaded. This case shows conclusively that the carbon-dioxide content of the bottles is not the feature of the closed bottles that determines whether or not a fly shall have Beaded wings. It leaves the question still unsettled as No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA 749 to the effect of moisture, but corresponds to the results obtained in the study of moisture effects. D. THE EFFECTS or ÅCIDITY AND ALKALINITY OF THE F'oop Normally the reaction of food at the time of putting it in the bottles is acid, the degree of acidity depending upon the length of time it has been fermenting. This sourness usually passes gradually away as the larve grow older, and by the time a brood begins to hatch the reaction is frequently quite alkaline, unless fresh food has been put recently into the bottle. On the other hand, if the acidity of the food is neutral- ized at the beginning with sodium hydrate or carbonate, or if the reaction is made alkaline while yet there remains a good deal of unfermented banana, the acidity will re- turn for a time if not carefully guarded against. There- fore to keep the reaction acid or alkaline is a difficult matter, and requires occasional stirring of the food to make the reaction uniform; this operation is likely to prove disastrous for the developing pupe. In the tests here recorded I used food that had been fermenting for one month, so that it had a very acid reaction that lasted till hatching time. For studies of the effect of alkalinity I used food that had been fermenting about one day and mixed with it sodium carbonate, sodium hydrate or ammonia. The results were unsatis- factory and the reaction did not remain constant in spite of my efforts, though on the whole it remained alkaline, and became strongly alkaline, and also slimy towards the end of the experiment, and not a great many flies hatched. I also used food that had not been allowed to ferment at all, and although I do not know its reaction, it was certainly not so alkaline as the last mentioned, nor so acid as the first. It was soon attacked by mold (Bread mold). I refer to it here as fresh food. The results are given in Table XX XI, but may be more briefly summarized here. Of 483 flies raised on sour food, 17 per cent. were Beaded. Of 605 flies raised on fresh food, 19.3 per cent. were Beaded. Of 317 flies raised on alkaline food, 38.1 per cent. were Beaded. 750 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII In other words, a high percentage of Beadedness came from flies raised on alkaline food, a low percentage from flies raised on acid food, and intermediate amount from flies raised on fresh food. A careful study of Table XXXI will reveal the curious partial sex-linkage of which I spoke on pages 15 et seq., and here, too, the explanation suggested there seems to apply as in other cases of the sort. It is not a little peculiar that in all of these food tests this phenomenon should have occurred, though I consider this purely a coincidence. In any case, if we can draw any conclusion at all from its appearance, it would only be that the re- action of the food has nothing to do with the occurrence of the phenomenon rather than the reverse. E. Tue Errects or RELATIVE TEMPERATURES No evident effect was produced by rearing the F, gen- eration in an ice-chest, but ratios were as varying as when the flies were raised at room temperature. Ratios of 15.4 per cent., 19.2 per cent., 10.3 per cent., 20 per cent. of Beaded offspring are examples of those given by broods raised at low temperatures. The cold does, however, lengthen greatly the larval life and flies were in the case of the brood last mentioned twenty-eight days in hatching. The brood consisted of 312 normal and 77 Beaded-winged flies, Similar results were obtained in experiments with heat, except that here the larval life was correspondingly shortened and was at times reduced to eight days. It was not found practicable to keep the flies at higher than 30°-33° Centigrade, as they soon died at higher tempera- tures. F. Tue Errects or Darkness Flies were raised in complete darkness and sister broods in full daylight, but no differences appeared in the offspring. Of 484 flies raised in darkness 30 per cent. had Beaded wings. Of 360 flies raised in the daylight, 29 per cent. had Beaded wings. This experiment seems No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA 751 to show conclusively that light and darkness do not influ- ence the.percentages of Beaded-winged flies. IV. THE EFFECT OF SELECTION ON THE PRODUCTION OF DIFFERENT TYPES OF BEADEDNESS Just how much can be accomplished by selection in Beaded stock was one of the first questions that arose. Morgan (1911a@) describes the origin of pure Beaded stock as having occurred through the selection of Beaded flies in the early generations after its first appearance. He says the first Beaded fly found arose in a culture of Droso- phila that had been exposed to radium. Mated to his sisters, 1.6 per cent. of the offspring were Beaded. When these Beaded flies were inbred 3 per cent. of the offspring were Beaded. These inbred gave 8.5 per cent. Beaded offspring. The same process continued through many generations has finally produced stock that gives in certain cultures nearly 100 per cent. Beaded wings. In continuing these selection experiments, he says more extreme forms of Beaded wings appeared, and at the time of publishing (March, 1911) he was attempting ‘‘to fix some of these extreme variations.’’ While engaged in this work other wing forms arose, most of which are among the best-known mutants of Drdsophila. Among these are Truncate, Miniature, Rudimentary, Vestigial and Balloon wings, and the Black and Yellow body colors. Most of these forms have been ‘‘purified’? now and Beadedness never appears in them though it can still be found in Vestigial stock. All of the above-named forms, by the way, with the exception of Truncate and Rudi- mentary bred true from the start. The Truncate case is not yet published and Rudimentary has proved (Morgan and Tice, 1914) to be due to a single Mendelian factor. The Rudimentary flies were at first self sterile and highly non-viable, and therefore gave peculiar results in breeding tests. When I first began work with Beaded flies (Sept., 1912) the stock gave 100 per cent. Beaded-winged offspring. 752 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII So soon that I did not realize it, nor think to count the generations, I had one stock that gave offspring much more extremely Beaded than the ordinary stock, and this stock is the one on which most of this report is based. About December, 1912, I started one stock bottle to form the basis for a ‘‘No selection ”’ test. The parents of this brood were ‘‘pure stock Beaded’’ males and females. The first generation, no normal-winged flies appeared. The generations following were made up by shaking at random from the bottle of the generation before a dozen or two flies into a new bottle. The second, third and fourth generations gave three normal-winged flies to 325 Beaded. The sixth, 3 normal to 100 Beaded. In later generations I occasionally found normal flies. The stock is in its 27th generation now, the 25th generation having given rise to a large brood of which I counted 541 flies (284 2 and 257 g), all of which had Beaded wings of a type averaging like those of Figs. 4-6. Itis very apparent that the stock is not under- going any marked change, though I can not guarantee that it would give exactly the same results in other respects as the extreme (selected) Beaded stock that I have used in the linkage tests. On the other hand, I have not been able thus far to increase the Beadedness of the selected stock beyond a point which it apparently reached many generations ago. The Figs. 1-12 (excepting 2 and 4), which are here re- produced, were made under Dr. Morgan’s direction long before I took up the work, and the forms he had drawn then are as extreme as any that I now have. If this extreme stock be allowed to go without eden for two or three generations, it ‘‘reverts’’ to a less ex- treme form, from which it can apparently be recovered by one mass selection. I feel confident that in selecting the extreme forms one merely selects a large percentage of individuals that are heterozygous for l, and of course when the stock is not selected for a while, LL forms be- come relatively more numerous. This would account for all the facts here recorded. No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA 753 On the other hand, selection for less extreme Beading is also rapidly effective and normal-winged forms appear soon, but this effect soon reaches its limit apparently, and a normal strain or even a strain throwing a high percentage of normals has not yet been obtained. I am not yet certain that it can not be done. I selected in each direction for eleven generations without marked success beyond that here recorded. V. MUTATION IN BEADED STOCK A, GENERAL STATEMENT As will be gathered from statements made in the last section, the Beaded stock has been prolific in giving muta- tions. There has been no especial attempt made to see how many different mutants could be obtained from the stock, and yet a goodly number have appeared. Most of these have been marked types showing little variation and coming out regularly and distinctly in Mendelian propor- tions in crosses with other types. They have in general bred true from the start without further selection. A few of these have been of a sort to confuse for a time the study that I have been making, because of their re- semblance to certain types of Beaded flies. The criterion in every case as to whether or not a fly was an ordinary Beaded fly or a new ‘‘mutant’’ was its genetic behavior, and the cases to be here described have, with the excep- tion of Stumpy, shown themselves to be due to a single gene conforming in general to those of other well-known mutants of Drosophila. B. Prrrect NorcHep Wines In the beginning of my work on Beaded wings I thought it might be possible to isolate definite types from the Beaded stock by crossing out to Wild and extracting the F, types that appeared; or by back-crossing the F, forms to Wild again and extracting new types, ete. Several thousand flies were raised in the hope of accomplishing this, but the ‘‘types’’ found did not breed true, but con- tinued to behave like ordinary Beaded flies, from whose 754 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vow. XLVIII many original types none were distinguishable. Finally a genuine new ‘‘type’’ appeared, with both wings alike and definitely ‘‘notched’’ (Fig. 13, p. 730). This female which was at sight named Perfect Notched, was mated to Wild. Her ancestry was as follows: : The grandmother came from pure Beaded stock, and the grandfather from Wild stock. Their offspring con- sisted of 18-Beaded and 69 normal flies. A Beaded female of this generation was mated to a normal brother and gave 100 Beaded offspring, male and female, and one ‘‘perfect notched’’ female. This female and her descendants behaved in a very different manner, genetically, than the Beaded stock from which she arose. She was mated to a Wild male and gave 62 Beaded off- spring and 112 normal offspring. Of the Beadeds, 50 were notched in a way resembling the parent and of the 50, 49 were females. Several other peculiar wing types appeared among the remaining 12 Beaded flies of this generation, but did not breed true and were later dis- carded. The notched male gave ordinary Beaded and normal offspring and never gave in either the first or later gen- erations any ‘‘notched’’ offspring. He was probably an extreme variant of a common Beaded type (Fig. 4). Of the normal offspring of the Perfect Notched female four pairs were made up. Seven hundred and forty-nine normal sons and daughters appeared, and no notched. Of the notched daughters of the perfect notched female, two were mated to normal brothers and two to Wild males. Their progeny was: Notched 9 Notched © Normal @ Normal g By normal brothers ...... 53 0 79 69 By wild males. :......-.: 56 0 47 46 Six of the notched females of this generation were mated to normal brothers and gave Notched 9 Notched @ Normal 9 Normal g 126 0 144 120 At this time, June, it was necessary to leave New York. _ In traveling, the Perfect Notched stock was lost. Enough No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA "(155 had, however, been done to show definitely the nature of the mutation involved. It is clear that the perfect notched wings owed their appearance to a dominant sex-linked gene, lethal for males. This accounts for the fact that the males are only half as numerous as the females, and none of them notched, while notched and normal females occur in nearly equal numbers. It also accounts for the fact that the normal females of these generations gave no notched offspring. Other sex-linked lethal genes have appeared from time to time in the crosses of Beaded flies with others, but none of them were dominant, and therefore they made themselves evident only by preventing the development of one half of the males. I have not worked out the inheritance of these cases. C. Spreap Wines Comment has already been made on the extreme num- ber of wing types that appeared both in the F,, F,, and back-cross generations of the cross between Beaded and Vestigial flies. Most of these forms gave results too com- plex to be analyzed at present. However, among the off- spring of a considerable number of the F, females there were flies with wings perfectly normal in appearance save that they were held at right angles to the long axis of the body. In all, 60 flies with Spread wings appeared. One of the 60 had wings very slightly Beaded. Some of them were mated together and produced only spread-winged offspring with no sign of Beadedness. Spread-winged males were mated to Pink Black females in order to test the linkage of Spread. (Pink is in the third chromosome group, and Black in the second.) The F, generation gave only flies with red eyes, gray bodies and normal wings (neither Spread nor Beaded). In the F, generation were Black flies, Gray flies, and Red-eyed flies with normal and with Spread wings, but none of the Pink-eyed flies had Spread wings, though a large number of F, Pink normal flies appeared. The Pink-eyed flies were also mated inter se, but no Spread-winged flies appeared in the F, generation. This definitely places the gene for 756 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII Spread wings in the third chromosome group. Beaded wings have not appeared in the stock bottles of Spread which breeds perfectly true. D. Stumpy Wines Recently a new non-lethal sex-linked character has ap- peared in the offspring of the cross of an F, Beaded male to a Wild female. Its nature has not yet been worked out, since only males have thus far appeared. The flies have wings resembling those of Vestigial, save that they are not held at right angles to the body, but in the normal position. Vestigial is not a sex-linked character. SuMMARY The character under consideration is that of Beaded wings in Drosophila ampelophila. All gradations of form between that of normal wings (Fig. 1) and those shown in Figs. 2 to 12 oceur in the stock bottles, though certain selected strains of the stock give no normal- winged offspring. When a Beaded fly is mated to a fly of a stock not carrying genes for Beadedness in its germ plasm a vary- ing percentage of the F, offspring is Beaded. If the male parent is Beaded the majority of the Beaded offspring are usually females; and if the female parent is Beaded, the majority of the Beaded offspring are usually males. A female Beaded fly however gives a larger percentage of Beaded daughters than does a male Beaded fly. This phenomenon is repeated from generation to generation, no matter whether a given Beaded fly has come from a male or female Beaded parent, and this shows that the phenomenon is not caused by a sex-linked gene. This phenomenon is not caused by non-disjunction of a sex-linked gene, for tests of both the Beaded and Wild stocks showed non-disjunction to be a rare phenomenon. The only explanation suggested was that the male off- spring were somewhat influenced to or away from Beaded- ness by the nature of the cytoplasm that was brought in with the egg, while females were not readily influenced in pie way. oo . No. 576] VARIATION IN DROSOPHILA 757 A study of the F, generation shows that the majority of the normal F, offspring differ from the majority of the Beaded F, offspring genetically in that normals give fewer Beaded offspring in the F, generation than do the Beaded flies. Beaded wings showed no linkage to any sex-linked character. Approximately one half of the flies of the F, generation of a cross between Beaded flies and flies with characters whose genes were in the second chromosome, showed linkage in the following generation to second chromo- some characters, while one half of the flies did not show such linkage. The cases where linkage did not occur gave a slightly lower percentage of Beaded offspring than did those where linkage was present. An explana- tion of these phenomena is sought in the suggestion that there was in the second chromosome a gene, here called 1, that was recessive but that in the heterozygous condition intensified the dominance of another gene, called B’, which was not in the second chromosome. This gene 1 behaves as a lethal factor preventing the development of any fly that carries it in a homozygous condition. All of the F, offspring of the crosses of Beaded flies by flies with characters caused by genes in the third chromosome showed linkage in the following generation between Beaded wings and the third chromosome char- acters. This was taken to signify that there was in the third chromosome a non-lethal gene concerned in the development of Beaded wings. This gene was called B’. This gene was shown to be the essential germinal factor in the production of Beaded wings. It is sometimes dominant and sometimes recessive. The determination as to whether B’ should be dominant or recessive seems to lie in several possibilities: 1st, the nature of the egg cytoplasm; 2d, the presence or absence of the gene l; 3d, the nature of the environmental con- ditions. With reference to environmental conditions, it was shown that a larger percentage of the F, generation had Beaded wings when the culture was wet than when it was 758 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vov. XLVIII dry; and more when the food was alkaline than when it was acid. No other environmental factors were discov- ered which influenced the production of Beaded wings. Selection of more or less extreme Beaded flies very quickly moves the average Beadedness of the offspring in the direction of the selection, but this selection appar- ently becomes further ineffective in a very few genera- tions. Mutation is of very frequent occurrence in the Beaded stock and the new mutants obtained have in most cases shown themselves to be produced under the influence of one normally Mendelizing gene. I acknowledge with pleasure the kindly interest and suggestions made from time to time by Dr. A. H. Sturte- vant and Mr. H. J. Muller. These have been of much assistance to me. My thanks are also especially due to Dr. T. H. Morgan whose advice and criticisms at critical points have never failed to aid in clearing up the situation. BIBLIOGRAPHY Baur, E. 1912. Einführung in die experimentale Vererbungslehre siete C..B.° 1913. PE EET of the Sex Chromosomes of Dro- i Jour. Exp. Zool., 5. Hoge, M. A. 1914, The Talons of Temperature on the Development of a Mendelian Character. Jour. Exp. Zool. In press MacDowell, E. 1914. a Factors in Mendelian Inheritance. Jour. Exp. Baat, Vol. Morgan, T. H. 1911a. ah ore of Nine Wing Mutations in Drosophila. Science, N. S., Vol. 33. Morgan, T. H. 1911b. A year Sex-limited Character. Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. and Med., Vol. Morgan, T. H. 1912a. The teint of a Mendelian Result a the Influ- ence of the Environment. Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. and Med., Vol. gic bi H. 19126. The Rieaiaawibons of a New Sex-ratio in Dresbphita. Science, N. S., Vol. 36. aac 7. mi 1912. E Linkage in the Second Chromosome of the Male. Science, N. S., Vol. 36. Morgan, T. H. 19124. "i Modification of the Sex-ratio and of other Ratios in grays through Linkage. Zts. ind. Abst. u. Vererb., Bd. VII. Morgan, T. H., and Tice, S. C. 1914. The Influence of the Havivamaue on the Size of ‘Expected Classes. Biol. Bull., Vol. 26. Shull, G. H. 1914 Sturtevant, A, H "1913. A Third Group of Linked enag T in K Teisi tta ampelophila. aoe N. 8., Vol. 37. SHORTER ARTICLES AND CORRESPONDENCE ON THE PROGRESSIVE INCREASE OF HOMOZYGOSIS BROTHER-SISTER MATINGS Ir has been brought to my attention that the note concerning inbreeding, written at the request of Mr. Phineas W. Whiting to add to his paper on ‘‘ Heredity of Bristles in the Common Green- bottle Fly, Lucilia Sericata Meig.,’’ which appeared in the AMER- ICAN NATURALIST for June, 1914, might be taken to mean that my data had been sent by Dr. E. M. East to Dr. Raymond Pearl by whom it had been published as his own. I wish to make it clear by a statement of the facts herewith that no such interpretation should be placed upon the note. I was seriously ill at the time and did not submit my manuscript to Dr. Castle or Dr. East for revision, as I should ordinarily have done. In that case no doubt, any ambiguity of statement would have been pointed out to me. ; Mendel, in his original paper, showed that if equal fertility of all plants in all generations is assumed, and, furthermore, if every plant is always self-fertilized, then in the nth generation the ratio of any allelomorphic pair (A,a) would be 2”—1 AA: 2 Aa:2"--laa. This statement was generalized in 1912 by East and Hayes! for any number of allelomorphie pairs. ‘‘The prob- able number of homozygotes and any particular class of heterozy- gotes in any generation r is found by expanding the binomial [1 + (2"—1)]" where n represents the number of character pairs involved. The exponent of the first term gives the number of heterozygous and the exponent of the second term the number of homozygous characters.’’ A little later Jennings independ- ently showed how homozygotes are produced from heterozygotes by self-fertilization.? East and Hayes? published no generalized formula for calcu- lating the reduction toward homozygosis through any other type of mating, but that this was thought to be a proper conclusion deducible from the above is shown by the following quotation (p. 21): 1U. S. Dept. Agr., Bur. Plant Ind., Bull, No. 243. 2 AMER. NAT., August, 1912. 3 Loc. cit. 759 760 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVIII Close selection, of course, tends toward the same end (homozygosis), but not with the rapidity or certainty of self-fertilization. This idea is further shown by their statements under the head- ing ‘‘Extension of Conclusions to the Animal Kingdom”’ (pp. 39-43). i A little later Mr. Whiting had occasion to work out the results of random matings of brothers and sisters, in connection with his work at the Bussey Institution. He found that the amount of heterozygosis was reduced one eighth in matings of the F, gen- eration and from this concluded that the remaining heterozygosis was reduced one eighth in each succeeding generation, so that in the nth generation the number of matings which would produce at least some heterozygous offspring would be (7/8)"*. He showed these figures to Dr. East, who agreed with the general conclusion (tendency toward homozygosis), but thought that the ratio would not hold for offspring after the F, generation. Dr. East, however, after a casual examination was not able to show Mr. Whiting the fallacy in his work and did not go into the matter further. In the AMERICAN NATURALIST for October, 1913, Dr. Raymond Pearl criticized the extension of the conclusions for self-fertilized plants to the animal kingdom.* He applied the figures of Pear- son, 1904,5 for random matings, which show that the relative number of homozygotes and heterozygotes remains constant in a population where all factors of fertility, virility and environ- ment have the same effect upon each individual in each genera- tion. Dr. Pearl’s error, as he has since recognized, lies in the fact that in the F, generation random mating involves only brothers and sisters, while in all subsequent generations it also involves other ‘relationships. en I read Dr. Pearl’s article in October I naturally won- dered why there was such a difference of opinion between Dr. East, Mr. Whiting and Dr. Pearl. Before finishing the article I computed the amount of homozygosis in the F, generation as 0 per cent.; in F,, 50 per cent.; F, 50 per cent.; F,, 62.5 per cent., and F,, 68.25 per cent. As soon as possible after that I figured other generations until the heterozygosis would be reduced to one half of one per cent. of the maximum of heterozygosis in the 4 East and Hayes, 1912, loc. cit. 5 Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. (A), Vol. 203, pp. 59 and 60. No. 576] SHORTER ARTICLES AND CORRESPONDENCE 761 F, generation and found that this was accomplished in the F, generation, the amount of heterozygosis in each generation being: Fe, 75.000 per cent. Fs, 94.312 per cent. Fi», 98.710 per cent. F,, 79.687 per cent. Fy, 95.398 per cent. Fa, 98.956 per cent. Fs, 83.594 per cent. Fs 96.277 per cent. Fa, 99.155 per cent. Fə, 86.719 per cent. Fis, 96.988 per cent. F», 99.317 per cent. Fy, 89.258 per cent. Fin, 97.563 per cent. Fa, 99.447 per cent. Fu, 91.309 per cent. Fs, 98.029 per cent. Fæ, 99.553 per cent. F.» 92.969 per cent. F», 98.405 per cent. Fæ, 99.638 per cent. With the approval of Dr. Castle and Dr. East I prepared to pub- lish these figures. Shortly after this Dr. Pearl wrote to Dr. East asking for an opinion upon his article. Dr. East, in the meantime, by a method differing from mine, had worked out the ratios independently. Before answering Dr. Pearl’s letter, however, Dr. East compared his results with mine. They agreed. Dr. East then wrote to Dr. Pearl, giving a short rebuttal of Dr. Pearl’s arguments, enclosing some of his own figures and adding that a student of Dr. Castle’s (myself) was thinking of publishing the complete figures. Dr. Pearl immediately acknowledged his mistake and -very gener- _ ously asked if he should wait until I had published my article before he published a correction. Dr. East replied that he could see no, reason for delaying the correction and advised me of this reply. Since it seemed proper for Dr. Pearl to correct his previous article, I decided to withhold my own figures and incorporate them later in a paper bearing also upon other matters. Dr. Pearl’s second article came out in the AMERICAN NATURALIST for January, 1914, and this paper together with the third article in the same journal for June, 1914, shows that his work was en- tirely independent of Dr. East’s or my own. | When Mr. Whiting asked me for a note giving the figures showing what might be expected in the way of an automatic in- erease in homozygosity when brothers were mated with sisters in successive generations, as Mr. Whiting had done with his flies, I naturally was pleased to have him accept my figures as correct- ing his own, and at the same time give me an opportunity to ac- knowledge my indebtedness to those who furnished the idea upon which my figures were based. H. D. Fisa BUSSEY INSTITUTION, FOREST HILLS, Mass. August 18, 1914 NOTES AND LITERATURE MENDELIAN FLUCTUATIONS? WHEN the observed proportions, say of dominants and reces- sives, in any Mendelian experiment are worked out for small groups, such as individual litters or the seeds on individual plants in individual fruits, considerable fluctuations round the expected proportions may be observed. In the present note the magnitude of these fluctuations is compared with the magnitude to be ex- pected if the fluctuations were the result merely of chances of sampling—corresponding to the fluctuations that would be ob- served in drawing, say, samples of black balls from a bag con- taining white and black balls in the proportion of 3 to 1. In so far as there is good agreement, this is additional confirmation of the Mendelian process holding good in its simplest form: if the fluctuation observed is markedly greater than this theory would indicate, some source of disturbance is certainly present, but whether this disturbance arises from irregularities in the distribution of the gametes or merely from extraneous circum- stances (varying death-rates or difficulties of sorting) can not, of course, be determined from the data alone. For albinos in individual litters of mice (Darbishire’s data), and for numbers of ‘‘green’”’ or ‘‘wrinkled’’ in Mr. Bateson and Miss Killby’s crosses of peas I find exceedingly good agreement, at least if very small plants are omitted. Lock’s data for maize give good agree- ment for the DRX DR cross, but poor agreement for the DR X RR cross. Some data given me by Miss E. R. Saunders for seed characters in the individual fruits of stocks show rather irregular results. Further comparisons on similar lines would be of interest, especially for the DR X RR cross, for which very few data are available. For the case to afford a good test the sorting should be clear and there should be nothing in the data to suggest differential death rates obviously. GUY. 1¢‘¥Fluctuations of Sampling in Mendelian Ratios,’’ G. Udny Yule (Proe. sci no dows, Soc., XVII, 425). 762 INDEX NAMES OF CONTRIBUTORS ARE PRINTED IN SMALL CAPITALS Age, Mean, at Marriage of Men and Women, Variation and Cor- relation, J. ‘Physiological Climatic Reae FREEMAN, 356 Atlelomorphism, Multivle, W. CASTLE, Allelomorphs Multiple, in Mice, T MoRGaN, 449; and 0 , GLOVER M., Pattern Devel- opm ent in Mammals aud Birds, 885, 467, 550 Analysis of a Case ariation in sophi ag paad of its Linkage Balarins, N S. DEXTER and Plent, ” sessile and RD, 641 Ants, Gynandromorphous, described during the a ade a Pe WILLIAM MORTON WHE Apple, A We of Variation | in pan Wade: 595 Apterous Prosenik and its thie 67 p- sella Bursa-pastoris, HENRI Hus, 193 erage H. L., The Meadow Jump- Mouse (Za apus Hudsonius) porny regarding Hibernation, Behavior, Genetic, An Pite a Drosophila and its, CHARLES W. METZ, 67. Bessey, CHaRLES E., A Study of Desert Figetation £ Biology of the Thysanoptera, A. lopm , 885, 467, 530 Br reeding, Experiments = Grass- hoppers, Nabours’s, TETI, Ken 317; Alfalfa, Se 763 ARTHUR HARRIS and | | Primula E. | Bristles, Heredity of, Gre Close Li nk- | The p at Distinction | M c Wi e Goa | “Ye Climatie F. FREEMAN, Correlations ctions in, GEO. B., The Chromo- sis of Linkage ap- | ical | Rea | 356 | BRIDGES, CALVIN | | some "Hypothe plied to Ciao in Sweet Peas and e Com een-bottle wry, pike HITING, British Islands, ° The Endemic Mam- mals of the, T. D. A. COCKERELL, 177 | | | | | | | | | | | Calf, Double- ig a Osteology of, . M. | Capsella pe arachnoidea, | The Origin of X, Henri Hus, 193 | CasE, E. estoration a Edapa- osaurus crueiger Cope, 1 | CASTLE , Some New Nase | of Rats and Gann -pigs and their | relation to Problems of Color In- heritance, 65; Yellow Varieties of Rat Multiple Allelomorphs and Close Linkage, 503 bil- Castle and Phillips on the Varia | ity of Genes, The Bearing of the | Selection Eo iai of, HEr- . MULLER, 567 Asa C., The Effect of | eg of Distribution on Specia- - tion, 129 ! | Chromosome Hypothesis of Linkage esate to Cases in Peas rimula, CALVIN B. BRIDGES, Citrus, Hybrids, Swingle on Varia- tion in F,, and the ry of Zy- Clima tic Rosetions | in Alfalfa Breed- ing, GEO. F. CocKERELL, T. D. A , The \demi Mammals of the British Islands, 177 764 CoLLINs, G. N. and J. H. KEMPTON, Inheritance ot Endosperm Tex- a in t X Waxy Hybrids of Mai 584. Color p Re Some New Vari- eties of s and Guinea-pigs, and yas Relation to Problems of, 6 CooK, 2 Marad relating to Gen- eric Types, 308 Correlation, ‘and Variation in the Mean Age at Marriage of Men Women, J. ae HARRIS Roxana H. Viv 5 Correlations, Physiclogtea and Cli- tic Reactions in Alfalfa Breed- ae EO. F. ; Porapon ies and Shorter Articles, 122, 177, 248, 308, 383, 446, 191, 567, 635, 693, 759 Davis, BRADLEY Moore, Stomps’s Œnothera ep ., 498 Deri Vegetat A Study of, sophila by a oon y of its Linkage Relations, 712 Differential Mortality with Respect to Seed Weight occurring in Field RTHUR HARRIS, aes ip in bhatar nar Galton a GGLES GATES, 697 Distribution. The Effect of Extent ho & irds and Mammals, GRINNELL, 248; A Study of Pact- tors governing, "Punzas E. WHr ING, 33 ‘‘ Dominant’? ‘t Recessive”? Spotting in Mice, C. C. LITTLE, 74 Drosophila, The Redupication Hy- pothesis as applie NT, 535; hn y E i tions, JOHN S. TER, Duplicate venes, SEWALL WRIGHT, 638 E. M., and H. produced ments vith Tobacco, 5 K. HAYES, A e Chan THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vor. XLVIII genap of Mammals, VERNON N KELLOGG, 257 apioa “eas lone Res- s 117 he “Inheritance of ing Somat e Variation in ze, 8 OLLINS and J. H KEMPTON, 584 Environmental Work, Humidity—a Neglected Factor, FRANK LUTZ, Errors, Probable, A Short-cut in the omputation of Certain, HOWARD F Ether, The Fa ilure of, to produce Mutations in Dros ophila, ToB. FERIER “705 Evolution, and Taxonomy, X, 369 IsH, H. D., On the Progressive In- crease of Homozygosis Brother X Sister Matings, 758 Fluctuations, Mendelian, G. UDNY YULE, 764 Fly, tha Common Green-bottle, oa redity ra ote tles in, PHINE W. WHI Formule for. the Results of Inbreed- ing, H. S. JENNINGS, 6 FREEMAN, Physiological Correlations and Reaz tions in Alfalfa Breeding, 356 FROST, rt-cut in the paige | of Certain Prob- able Errors, 696 GATES, R. RUGGLES, Galton and Dis- continuity in Variation, 697 eg AE cane, Terms relating to, O. 00K Genes, The Paria ni x5 see geting Experiments of Cas Phil- - on the Variability pa ANN J. MULLER, 567; Du ists, BEWA WRIGHT, 638 Genetic, Analysis of the Changes produced by saat in Experi- pret with Tobacco, ST Genetics, aryen Terminologies . E. Cas 83 in, GERO EROULD, JOHN T ’ Species-building A Hybridization and Mutation, No. 576] Grasshoppers, Nabours’s racer = iments with, JoHN 8 31 Gregory” s ie i oAinter tera A ess de Sei egation in, Hark NN J. on, , 508 Gant. JOSEPH, Pania to Dis- tribution as regards Birds and Mammals, 248 Gataka bigs, and Rats, Some New Varieties and their Relation to Probl ea of Color Inheritance, W. E. CASTLE, 6 GULICK, sous T., Isolation = Se- lection allied in Principle, eA D 1903-1913, WILLIAM MORTON WHEELER, 49 HAGEDOORN, A. C. and A. Another Hypothesis to account for Dr. Swingie’s Experiments wit Citrus, 440 HAGEDOORN, A. L., Repulsion in Mice, 6 Harris, J ae Ugeran eed Correlation in the Marrıage of Men and Women, 635 s, H. K., and E. M. East, Genetic "Analysis of the Changes produced by Selection in Exper ments with Tobacco, ‘The Influence of onarchs ode E; 265; of Bristles in the Common reen- bottle Fly, PHINEAS W. WHITING, Hibernation, tig on the Meadow Jumping Mouse, L. H. BABCOCK, 485 r Ti Brother-Sister Ma- The Progressive Increase Fisu, 758 D. Hamidiiy—a N eglected Factor in Work, FRAN Environ K E. , + Luz, Hus, gee The Origin of X Cap- pec a-Pastoris Arachnoidea, Hurcueson, T. B., Th — Years of Wheat Selection, 4 ph ridization and "Station, Spe- aa x Mendelian Population, RAYMOND PEARL, 57; Notes on INDEX 765 RAYMOND PEARL, 491; and Rela- COLL Internal Relations of Terrestrial i. sociations, ARTHUR G. VES 1 Tsolation and Selection F in Principle, JoHn T. GULICK, 63 JENNINGS, H. S., Formule fi the Results ‘of Inbreeding, 693 Woods on Heredity and horas ‘Influence of Monarchs,’’ 255 KELLOGG, VERNON gerry Ectopar- asites of Ma mmals, MPTON, and Aa X. COLLINS, Inheritance of Endosperm Tex- ure in Sweet X Waxy Hybrids of Maize, 584 Linkage, in the Silkworm Moth, A. H. STURTEVANT, 315; and Mis- one Terminologies in Genetics, W. CasTLE, 383; Close, r T. Continu sophila by a Study at its, JOHN S. DEX 711 Literature and Notes, 185, 255, 315, 50 62 ’ and < E., Humidity—a Ne- ected F Factor in Environmen tal ork, Maize, The Inheritance of a Recur ring Somatic Variation in Varie- gated Ears of, R. A. E 766 87; Inheritance of Endosperm Texture in Sweet X Waxy Hy- brids of, G. N. COLLINS and J. H. KEMPTON, 584 Mammals, The Endemie, of the Brit- ish Islands, D. KERELL, 177; and birds, Barriers to Dis- RINNELL, 248; tern Development in, GLOVER ALLEN, 385, 467, 550 Marriage of Men and W. omen, Vari- and Correlation the Matings, Homozygosis Brother X pees, The Eropa Tans -H D. Fisu, 759 Maiioi Janion Mouse (Zapus Hudso n especially see Hibernation, H. L. -BAB 5 en ap Population, Oa. the Re- nbreeding a, RAYMOND Fluctuations, G. s. W., An Apterous Drosophila and its Genetic Be- havi Mice, A ponani and ‘‘Recess- ive,’’ Spo TTLE, tting in, C. U. 74; Multiple Allelomorphs in, T. H. Morean, 449; Repulsion in, A. ; RN, 699 ‘í Monarchs, The Influence of,’’ and eredity, V. L. K., 255 Morgan, T. H Multiple Allelo- s in T inked I 577; The Fail- = of Ether to pebaae Mutations , 705 Mortali ity, rabaan with respect o Seed W t occurring in Field Cier of Pisum sativum, J. AR- THUR Moth, Silkworm, ys in the, A. L STURTEVANT, Motile and Sessile ine and Ani- | mals, comparison of the Re- sponses of, VICTOR E. SHELFORD, e of the Selecti ion ` Experiments Z | a THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. XLVII of Castle and rio on the Triru: ity of Genes, 56 Alle Eier ee Multiple, WwW. E. CASTLE, 383; -Allel phs in Mice GAN W Mutation, and Hybridization, Spe- cies- e by, JoHN H. GER OUL Mutations, se in @Œnother biennis P Y M Dii sophila, The Failure of Ether to Produce, T. H. Moraan, 705 Nabours’s, Breeding Experiments i Grasshoppers, JOHN S. DEX- rasshoppers, W. E. Fig 383 Notes ane Literature, 185, 255, 315, 505, 639, 762 Œnothera — L., Parallel Muta- cares = J. pe 494; SH Dana Osteology “of 2 Double headed Calf, . M. REESE, 701 Pattern Development in Mammals and Birds, GLOVER M. ALLEN, "385, 467, ARL, RAYMOND, On the Results of ionship ppa aa 513 proes. and Castle, Selec Experi im aoa Variability 4 Genes, on HERMANN Physiological’ ar i and Cli- aeg etions in Alfalfa Breed- g, GEO. F N, 356 Pisum pe EN Differential Mortal- with Resp o Seed eigh po en in. Pela Onara of, J. ARTHUR HARRIS, Plants and Animals, Sessile and Mo- tile, A Comparison of the Re- E. SHELF sponses of, VICTOR ORD, Primulas, Tetraploid, vie bel s, A Segregation in, HERMANN J. MULLER, So Rats and Guinea-pigs, Some New arieties of, and their labian to Problems of See ene Inheritance, W. Fe w Varieties of, W. E. CASTLE, 254 ‘í Recessiv 6 and ‘í Dominant,’ No. 576] Spotting in Mice, C. C. LITTLE, 74 Reduplication Hypothesis