THE AMERICAN NATURALIST THE AMERICAN NATURALIST A MONTHLY JOURNAL DEVOTED TO THE ADVANCEMENT OF THE BIOLOGICAL SCIENCES WITH SPECIAL REFERENCE TO THE FACTORS OF EVOLUTION VOLUME LHI NEW YORK THE SCIENCE PRESS 1919 a AA n f THE AMERICAN NATURALIST Vot- DIIIL. January, 1919 No. 624 THE INHERITANCE OF HULL-LESSNESS IN OAT HYBRIDS! PROFESSOR H. H. LOVE anp G. P. McROSTIE (IN COOPERATION WITH THE OFFICE OF CEREAL INVESTIGATIONS, U. 8S. DEPARTMENT OF AGRICULTURE) Tue purpose of this paper is to set forth some results obtained in certain crosses between the hull-less types of oats and some of the hulled forms. The hull-less types belong to the species Avena nuda. The origin of these forms is not definitely known, al- though, according to Carleton,? they appear to have come from central and eastern Asia. Reports are to the effect that a certain form of this oat has been cultivated in China for a thousand years or longer. These types are not generally cultivated in any other countries and perhaps due to this fact little study has been made of the various hull-less types and their possible origin. From some observations made on our material it seems quite possible that these hull-less forms may have originated through mutative changes. For example, in a pure line of the variety Sixty Day certain spikelets suddenly ap- peared which were very similar to the true hull-less forms in having the caryopsis loosely held in the glumes and an increased number of flowers per spikelet. These seeds were tested, but did not reproduce this hull-less tendency. A large number of similar cases have been ob- served particularly with hybrids, although none of these 1 Paper No. 68, Department of Plant Breeding, Cornell University, Ithaca, N: y. 2‘*The Small Grains,’’ 1916. 6 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LITI has been tested as to its inheritance as yet, but it is planned to do so. Trabut® says in regard to the possible origin of hull- less oats that ‘‘the study of the domestication of Avena presents, from the genetic point of view, some rather substantial arguments in favor of an ambient medium, a modifying agent causing fluctuations which end in the formation of varieties well characterized and fixed by selection.’’ | Previous Work A number of investigators have studied hybrids be- tween varieties of Avena nuda and Avena sativa. Nor- ton‘ was the first investigator in America to hybridize these forms. He reports that the spikelet of the naked varieties usually has more than three grains, while in the hulled types three grains is the limit. The first generation plants produced a head naked at the top and hulled at the bottom. In the second generation, one fourth of the progeny were typical naked plants, one fourth were hulled, and one half like the first generation hybrids. The naked plants all had long spikelets with more than three grains, while the hulled plants had spikelets with the usual two or three grains. In future generations no exception to this rule has been found except that one second generation plant of a cross between European Hull-less and Garton’s Tartar King which seems to have become fixed in the intermediate hybrid type. In this example we have an extremely rare case of the fixation of a heterozygote or hybrid type. Gaines® reports having made some hybrids between hulled and hull-less oats and first separated the F, types into two groups, hulled and hull-less. In making such a grouping the heterozygous types were put into the group which it resembled most. He obtained from one cross 48.7 per cent. hulled and 51.3 per cent. hull-less plants and says, ‘‘this indicates an intermediate about half- way between hulled and hull-less for the heterozygous types. As was mentioned above, the separation was made arbitrarily into hulled and hull-less, according to the type any given plant most nearly resembled.’’ 3 Journal of Heredity, Vol. 5, p. 84, 1914. Translation of original paper. 4 American Breeders’ Association, Vol. III, p. 285, 1907. 5 Washington Agr. Expt. Sta. Bul. 135, p. 58, 1917. No. 624] INHERITANCE OF HULL-LESSNESS 7 In another cross Gaines found 77.1 per cent. hulled to 22.9 per cent. hull-less, which caused him to conclude that the two crosses were not similar in their behavior. This would seem to be the case from the data at hand, yet in 1914 Gaines made a number of other crosses, among which according to the pedigree numbers is another one between these two sorts, Black (Wash. No. 665) and Hulless (Wash. No. 680) which gave, this time, results very similar to all the other crosses reported as made that year, which indicated a 1:2:1 ratio. Gaines did not offer any explanation as to the different behavior of these two crosses between the same two sorts. From these experiments Gaines concludes, the percentage of hulled type suggests a simple Mendelian recessive although in every case there are a few too many hulled plants. The percentage of hull-less plants is not only very irregular in the different erosses but is also irregular in the different families within the same cross with the exception of the two families of Sixty Day X Hull-less, which gave a ratio approaching 1:2:1. The intermediate types showed great variation. Plants could be found with only one or two spikelets that showed the hull-less character. Others could be found that showed the hulled character in only one or two spikelets, and plants were ob- tained with every degree of hull-lessness between these extremes. How- ever, most of the intermediates produced more than half hulled oats. A eurve fitted to these intermediate variations in Black Tartarian X Hull-less shows larger numbers at either extreme and few numbers showing per cents. of hulled oats ranging from 30 to 50. This is just the opposite of what we would expect if the hull-less character was caused by a single Mendelian unit which produced an intermediate in the F,. In a paper by Zinn and Surface® results are given of a cross between a hull-less and hulled oat. The sorts used were Avena sativa patula var. Victor, and Avena sativa nuda var. inermis. The results indicate that their forms agree very closely with those reported by Norton and Gaines. The following paragraph gives part of their conclusions. The F, generation is distinctly intermediate in most characters. In regard to the glumes, both naked and firmly hulled grain as well as in- termediate forms are found on the same panicle and even in the same 6 Journal of Agricultural Research, Vol. X, No. 6, pp. 310-311, 1917. 8 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vov. LIII spikelet. As shown in Table I, the spikelets near the top of the panicle are either entirely naked or nearly so, while those spikelets near the base of the panicle tend to be firmly hulled. A similar but less marked relation is to be observed between the spikelets at the tip and base of each whorl. In the F, generation a large number of intermediate forms appear. In addition to the two parental hull types, four intermediate classes were distinguished. These intermediate forms contain all gradations from the plants with perfectly hulled grain to the perfectly naked forms. As shown in Table II, the inheritance of the hull characters presents a simple Mendelian relation giving 1 hulled, 2 intermediate, 1 naked. Likewise, in respect to grain color, there are 3 black plants to 1 white, in the second generation. MATERIAL AND METHODS In connection with some experiments in oat breeding a number of hybrids between the hulled and hull-less forms have been made. While in these crosses the in- heritance of other characters such as color of glumes, pubescence, awns and the like, are very interesting, the present paper will be confined to the discussion of the inheritance of the hull-less and hulled characteristics. A more complete discussion of the various characters is being prepared for a later publication. e authors want to take this opportunity to express their appreciation for the valuable assistance in note- taking and tabulation of results rendered by W. T. Craig and Miss A. M. Atwater. Their work has been of great aid in conducting these experiments. The hull-less oat used for the various hybrids was typical of the Avena nuda group and differs from the Avena sativa forms by three important characters: (1) The lemma, or flowering glume, and palet do not clasp the kernel as in other forms, and the kernel is therefore loose, or free, within the hull; (2) The rachille of the three to many-grained spikelet are so elongated that the uppermost grains are borne above the empty glumes; (3) The glumes and the lemmas are similar in texture. The illustration (Fig. 1) will give a fair idea of the par- No. 624] INHERITANCE OF HULI-LESSNESS \ fare hull-lege sprkelet Fic. 1. THE FEMALE PARENT (Avena nuda) USED IN SERIES 382. ticular characteristics which separate Avena nuda from the hulled species. This type is typical of the hull-less forms used in the following hybrids. The hulled forms were all varieties of Avena sativa with one exception. In this case Avena fatua was used À l Ñ A Vy he f pone”: AMANN Fig. 2. THE MALE PARENT (Avena sativa) USED IN SERIES 382. as the hulled parent. Fig. 2 shows the hulled variety, Sixty Day, used in one of the crosses. It is typical of a hulled oat. The first crosses of this sort were made in 1910 and the F, plants grown in the greenhouse the following win- ter. The first cross was between Hulless and Black Tar- No. 624] INHERITANCE OF HULL-LESSNESS 11 tarian. Here the hull-less form was used as the female parent. The male parent possessed, as indicated by its name, black glumes, and was a typical hulled oat. The other cross was between Danish Island and Hulless in which the hulled type was used as the female parent. In each case the F, type was typical of the F, types as de- scribed by the authors mentioned above. This form is intermediate in that both kinds of kernels, hulled and hull-less, are found on the same head. The type of pan- icle resembles the hull-less parent more than it does the hulled and may be considered as intermediate in type. There are some spikelets with hulled and some with hull- less kernels and also some with both hulled and hull-less kernels. As a usual thing the hulled spikelets occur towards the base of the panicle while the hull-less kernels occur near the terminal spikelet which is almost inva- riably hull-less if such kernels are present at all in the panicle. There are fewer hulled than hull-less kernels on the F, types. The percentage of hulled kernels does not usu- ally run very high. In Fig. 3 is shown a typical form of F, panicle of a cross between a hulled and hull-less oat. The F, generation of these two crosses were grown in the field in the summer of 1913. The plants were then sorted into two groups, hulled and hull-less, or hull-less like. All those plants having any indication of hull-less- ness were placed in the hull-less class. The result of these counts was as follows: Bocce a Varieties Crossed Hull-less | Hulled 111 Hulless X Black Eg aig Si Se rer ne. 129 37 51 Danish Island X Hulless. .:. 2... eo cs: 364 93 - 493 130 Although the ratio deviates considerably from 3:1 it indicates that this character behaves as a simple mono- hybrid and that there is one factor pair concerned. In 12 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Vou. LIII Fie. 3. F, INTERMEDIATE TYPE OF A CROSS BETWEEN HULL-LESS AND HULLED. Series 382, order to test this out more fully all plants from which good seed could be obtained were grown in the following year. The results obtained from these plants showed without doubt that the segregation followed a simple monohybrid ratio. That is, the pure hulled and hull-less plants bred true to these characteristics, while the inter- No. 624] INHERITANCE OF HULL-LESSNESS 13 mediate types reproduced the three types again. The second generation plants tested as to their composition gave the following results in the third generation: Seri Pure . Inter- rl Varieties Crossed Gelies |. mediat, | Hulkias lil Giuliess X Black Tartarian Iii. SEn 37 85 38 51 Danish island X Hualless -norak 115 216 114 152 301 152 It is apparent from these results that certain plants were classed as intermediate in the second generation, which were in reality pure hulled plants. It is evident that the hulled-hull-less character is inherited in a simple Men- delian fashion so far as its general behavior is con- sidered. The heterozygous plants produced in the third gen- eration were examined as to the relative amounts of hulled and hull-less kernels present. This was done by threshing a representative head from each plant by hand and counting the hulled and naked kernels and express- ing the result as the percentage of hulled kernels. In order to determine whether the results from a single head fairly represented the type of the plant a number of plants were examined and recorded a head at a time. The percentage of hulled kernels for a represen- tative head was then compared with that for the entire plant. The average percentage was the same for the results from single heads as it was for the entire plant. Although there was some deviation in the individual de- terminations, the correlation between the two methods is very high. The result of determining the percentage of hulled kernels was to indicate the great variation existing, which was from a very low to a very high percentage. As a result of these observations it was apparent that while in the hybrids under consideration the usual 1:2:1 ratio was observed, some factor or factors were pres- 14 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LII ent which affected the heterozygous forms in such a way as to modify the amount of hulled or hull-less kernels present. In order to determine this effect in a more definite way it was planned to sow seed from heterozygous individuals . which differed as to the percentage of hulled kernels present. A rather large number of such seeds were planted in 1915. The plants were severely injured by a storm, so that accurate pereentage determinations could not be made. In the meantime, however, a number of other crosses had been made in which the Avena nuda was used as one parent. The following sorts were crossed with the naked oats: Swedish Select, Sixty Day and Avena fatua. Other crosses are being studied but these will be reported on later. As regards the hull-less character the F, individuals of these crosses were all similar to the description of the first generation given earlier in this paper. Regarding the other characters, the cross between the hull-less form and Avena fatua showed some very interesting varia- tions. These will not now be discussed. Seeds of these various F, plants were sown and the resulting plants harvested. From each plant a head was saved and threshed separately by hand and the plants then sorted into hulled, intermediate and hull-less. The result of the several crosses is given here: Series Inter- | Hull- wa. Varieties Crossed Watled metae) tens 379 | Hulless X Avena fatua...... Mee bebe E ses 68 111 78 202° | Einlleds X Bwediush Select’... ffs bt Al 90 36 382 | Hollós X Bitty Dav. 6 ieee 75 193 53 Observed ois as A akara 184 394 | 168 Expotod aaae a 186.5! 373 186.5 The probable error is +7.98 and the observed num- bers agree fairly well with the expected numbers. The number of hull-less plants is too low and the number of intermediates too high. It is possible that in some cases No. 624] INHERITANCE OF HULL-LESSNESS 15 oR Fic. 4. HETEROZYGOUS TYPE F, POSSESSING 10 Per CENT. OF HULLED KERNELS. Series 382. hull-less plants may have been recorded as intermediates although the error from this source is not large. When the results are considered on a 1:3 basis and the hull-less and intermediates are grouped together we find that there is a percentage of 24.66 +1.07 hulled plants. 16 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. LIL aa Fic. 5. HETEROZYGOUS TYPE Fy POSSESSING 87.9 PER CENT. OF HULLED KERNELS. Seri 82. The results of these different hybrids show that hull- lessness is inherited in a simple monohybrid manner and that without doubt the difference between hulled and hull-less oats in this regard is represented by one pair No. 624] INHERITANCE OF HULL-LESSNESS 17 of factors. An analysis of the different heterozygous or intermediate individuals of these second generation plants showed that for these hybrids also there was a great amount of variation in the percentage of hulled or hull-less kernels in the individual plants, the variation ranging all the way from less than 5 per cent. to 95 per cent. or more. In Figs. 4 and 5 are shown two forms of heterozygous plants, one very low and one very high, in percentage of hulled kernels. These percentages were obtained from the heterozygous individuals by sorting the kernels from one head of each plant into hulled and hull-less as outlined earlier. The percentage of hulled kernels on the heterozygous plants of the second genera- tion for the three series is given in Table I. TABLE I SHOWING PERCENTAGE OF HULLED KERNELS ON THE HETEROZYGOUS PLANTS OF THE SECOND GENERATION IN CROSSES BETWEEN HULLED AND HULL-LESS OATS. Percentage of Hulled Number! Varieties Crossed 19! 19] alala al 19/99! alag 9! 9] 8} 2} 9 > 7“ AN BRIAR AY AY Ae! Al Ye OO Hi Hw) oO) Oo] © 72.5 7 2 7. 2 97.5 379 |Hulless X Avena Nabe ak 2| 8) 5| 4| 4) 3| 4| 4| 7| 5) 5) 8) 4 202 |Hulless X Swed- ish Select ......|10} 4| 4| 3| 5| 8| 2| 1| 7| 6| 3) 4) 2| 5 382 |Hulless X Sixty Bee ee ces o o ao ~J — © p w — or ~J A g= pt 5 5 5) 4) 4| 6 6| 820/152221 (15/13/1711 15/1112 17/21/22 From this table it is seen that there is considerable dif- ference in the percentage of hulled kernels on the dif- ferent heterozygous plant. The range is from a very low percentage or one which indicates nearly all hull-less to a very high percentage or one which is nearly all hulled. There is no general grouping near the middle of the series, as might be expected with the exception of series 382. This may be due to lack of numbers or to a segregation of the different types which give percentages ranging from low to high without any tendency to group- ing. That it is not due to lack of numbers is probably 18 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vor. LIII borne out by the fact that in series 51, where over 900 plants of the third generation were sorted into the dif- ferent classes, there was no indication of a grouping near the middle classes, in fact, the slight indication of group- ing was near the lower values. The distribution is as follows: TABLE II SHOWING PERCENTAGE OF HULLED KERNELS IN THE HETEROZYGOUS PLANTS OF THE THIRD GENERATION IN A CROSS BETWEEN DANISH ISLAND AND A HULL-LESS OAT. Percentage of Hulled Kernels Frequency Percentage of Hulled Kernels Frequency 0— 4.9 89 50.0 —54.9 42 5.0 — 9.9 80 55.0 —59.9 48 10.0 — 14.9 60 60.0 — 64.9 43 15.0 —19.9 49 65.0 —69.9 26 20.0 —24.9 59 70.0 —74.9 35 25.0 —29.9 48 75.0 —79.9 33 30.0 —34.9 47 - 32 35.0 —39.9 53 85.0 —89.9 26 40.0 —44.9 52 90.0 —94.9 21 45.0 —49.9 46 95.0 —99.9 15 It was planned to carry some of this work further to answer in general two questions which are: (1) Does the percentage of hulled plants obtained from any hetero- zygous parent vary with the percentage of hulled kernels possessed by that parent? (2) Do the hulled and hull- less kernels of a heterozygous plant give approximately the same results in their offspring? In order to obtain data on these questions two of the series have been continued. The hull-less-Avena fatua series has not been carried further as yet but it is planned to do so. RESULTS FROM Serres 202—Swepiso SELECT X HULLESS The first series to be discussed is the Swedish Select- Hull-less cross. Seed from two hulled and two hull-less plants of thẹ second generation were grown in the third generation and each bred true to type. In addition to these plants twenty heterozygous plants were selected for planting. These varied as to the amount of hulled No. 624] INHERITANCE OF HULL-LESSNESS 19 kernels. The range was from 3.2 per cent. to 92.0 per cent. The number of seed was not large, therefore the number of plants obtained was not as large as desired, yet from the consistency of the results certain conclu- sions are justified. The offspring from these twenty plants were sorted into the three classes, hulled, inter- mediate and hull-less. The intermediate plants were again threshed and the percentage of hulled kernels de- termined. In Table ITI is given the percentage of hulled condition in the parent plant, the segregation into the three groups, the percentage of hulled kernels in the heterozygous off- spring, the grouping into hulled and hull-less and (where both hull-less and intermediate plants are grouped to- gether) the percentage of hulled plants with the prob- able error. TABLE III SHOWING SEGREGATION IN F, OF CERTAIN F, PLANTS TOGETHER WITH THE CENTAGE OF HULLED SEED IN PARENT TYPE AND THE AVERAGE PERCENTAGE IN THE HETEROZYGOUS OFFSPRING. Segregation Obtained from Plants Sown and Resulting Percentage of Hulled Kernels on Intermediate Forms. ste of i a Bire Hull- Heals | Hultet | atisto] aa | a Tatar: | tla | naes P" O te in Plants mediate mediate Offspring 202al-4....| 44.9 27 22 44.8 27 24.11 + 2 34 51 40 21.0 34 91 27.20 + 2.61 20.9 12.8 27 22.13 10 42 5 42 97 30.22 + 2.48 a. 92.0 42 62 17 52.6 42 79 34. x 22...) 65.1 13 36 7 60.6 13 43 23.21 + 3.90 25 90. 14 36 9 55.3 14 45 23.73 + 3.80 26 76.6 14 37 21 56.3 14 58 19.44 + 3.44 29 30. 12 19 15 44.1 12 34 26.09 + 4.31 31 56. 16 43 21 47.6 16 64 38 10.5 21 40 22, 21 66 24.14 + 3.13 40...| 82.4 19 33 19 53.6 19 52 26.76 + 3.47 46 16.7 14 24.7 14 42 51 56.6 41 10 47.1 25 51 60 74.3 T 10 4 46.0 7 14 33.33 + 6.37 s 3.2 10 26 32 22.7 10 14.71 + 3.54 TT 65.4 15 21 14 34.9 15 30.00 + 4.13 92 35.3 12 10 8 36.0 12 18 40.00 120 44.7 14 18 8 33.9 14 26 35.00 + 4.62 121 9.5 13 11 23 20.2 13 34 27.66 + 4.26 TOS.. 391 661 426 391 |1087 26.45 + .76 Expected. . 5| 739 | 369.5 20 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vor. LIII The results of the segregation into the three classes gave 391 hulled, 661 intermediate and 426 hull-less. Here the hull-less plants are too great in number, while the number in the intermediate class is too small. The ex- pected numbers are 369.5:739:369.5, with a probable error of +11.23. It is possible that some intermediate plants were classed as hull-less. Such a condition is pos- sible since some intermediates are found bearing only one or two hulled kernels, and if these should be lost through shattering, such plants would be classed as hull- less when in reality they are intermediates. When the grouping is made into the two groups, hulled and hull- less, it is seen that the 3 to 1 ratio is approximated very closely, as there are 391 hulled plants to 1087 hull-less, giving a percentage of 26.45 + .76 hulled. An examination of this table shows further that some of the families do not give ratios close to 1:2:1. This is true with regard to certain families particularly with certain of those coming from plants low in percentage of hulled, and some of those relatively high in this re- spect. The results of some of these families have been brought together in Table IV. TABLE IV SHOWING SEGREGATION OF OFFSPRING COMING FROM SOME INDIVIDUALS LOW OR HIGH IN THE PERCENTAGE OF HULLED KERNELS. Percentage of Hulled Segregation of Offspring Into Different Types. Family Num- Seed in Plants SHORTEN a Sown Hulled Intermediate Hull-less 6 25. 34 51 40 9 20.9 27 35 60 38 10.5 21 26 40 46 16.7 14 20 22 66 3.2 10 26 32 121 9.5 B 11 23 T i 119 169 217 10 83. 42 63 34 11 92 '42 62 17 25 14 36 9 26 76.6 14 ot 21 40 82.4 19 33 19 Tow se: 131 231 100 No. 624] INHERITANCE OF HULL-LESSNESS 21 In this table the plants from parents having 25 or less per cent. of hulled kernels give 119 hulled:169 inter- mediate: 217 hull-less. From this result it appears that those plants having a low percentage of hulled or high percentage of hull-less kernels tend to produce a rela- tively high number of hull-less plants. On the other hand, those plants having more than 75 per cent. of hulled kernels do not give results so striking. There are more hulled than hull-less plants, yet not strikingly so, and the hulled do not run higher than the intermediates. It may be, however, that the degree of hull-lessness as expressed by the percentage may influence the segrega- tion in the following generations. This can not be def- initely stated from this cross, and further evidence will be needed. TABLE V SHOWING THE RELATION BETWEEN THE PERCENTAGE OF HULLED KERNELS ON HE HETEROZYGOUS PARENT PLANTS AND THE PERCENTAGE OF HULLED KERNELS ON ITS OFFSPRING Percentage of Hulled Oats on Offspring Percentage of Hulled a 2 alealale alelalelealelelelalalaiale Oats on Plants @| a| $ Si í 8] F) S| 3] S$] 31S) 3] Si Fe) Si si 3 S 2/4) s) 3) 33,2222) 3)s) 52 223 )3 “S| Si 8| 8| S| 3| S| 3| S| $| S| Sie] £ 8] Si 8} 3 2 12, 3} | 3/1 H i 111) | 1/1) | 26 5 1| 1| 2 2) 2 1} 1 10.5 5| 2; 3i 6| 6| 1,1) 1) | 1 26 16.7 A3 | 3i 4) 21/2) | 2 1 20 20.9 910| 7| 4 1} 1 35 25.0 5112| 4! 6| 5| 2| 9| 2| 4| 1 1 30.7 1} 1} | 1 2| 2| 2| 2) 1| 1| 1| 1| 21 ) 35.3 a} inuiti 1 1 ) 44.7 UHI 2 4| 3 3} 2) ] } 44.9 4' 31 6, |3' 1| 6| 3| 2| 3| 4! 7| 7! 6| o| 6| 2 63 56.6 1} 3i t 3] 3, 2} 6 3} 3} 2} 3ks) 1) al ala) fal 56.8 5| 1| 1; 4| 3| 2| 2| 4| 2| 5| 6| 2| | 3|- 2| 1 43 65.1 2 3 6| 4| 2| 4 2| 6| 4) 1| 2| |36 4 1| | 1} 1} 1) 4 2 1 1 1} | 21 3 1} 1} | 1 1 1} 1} | 1/1 ) 76.6 1] 2! 3} 2} 1) 1) 3] 81 41 5} | al ale 1| 37 32.4 1| 1| 2| | 2| 3| 3| 4) 3| 3| 1| 1| 2| 2| 2| 1| 2 3 33.0 5| 1| 2| 8| 4| 5|. 2| 7| 5| 2| 5| 3| 3| 5| 2| 1 1 3 90.3 4 2) 2 1 3} | 31 1] 7| 11 5] 3] 3} 5 92.0 1| 4| 2| 1| 3) 3| 2| 3| 2| 5 5} 6| 4| 2| 4| 4| 7| 1| 3| | 62 50 60 37:39 41'39/39130 32136 39 42 32'34'18 32'32.15112] 2 661 22 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII The relation between the percentage of hulled kernels on the parent and the percentage of hulled kernels on the heterozygous offspring for this series is shown by the correlation table given above. The correlation coefficient is. 421 + .022, which shows a very definite relation be- tween the percentage of hulled in the parent and offspring. This relation is also better shown by means of a curve (Fig. 6) in which the parents are represented by the dotted line beginning with the lowest and increasing to the highest value. On the same ordinate is plotted the average value for the heterozygous offspring, and to this line has been fitted a straight line whose equation is y = 20.1999 + 1.95792. Certain of these families show a decided grouping; for example that represented by 20.9 per cent. shows a de- cided tendency to be grouped in the lower classes, while that represented by 65.1 per cent., with five exceptions, shows a grouping around the higher classes. Resvuuts oF Serres 382—Srixty Day X HULL-LESS From the second generation of this cross six hetero- zygous plants were selected for further study. The re- - sults of three of these will be discussed here. These plants possessed different amounts of hulled kernels, which expressed in percentages were as follows, 73.3, 37.7, 49.3. Thus, there was one high, one low and one medium plant. The offspring of these gave the follow- ing results when grouped in the three classes: Hulled Intermediate Hull-less A E 23 ! 55 20 t 15 37 24 ao oo 53 105 49 91 197 93 These figures agree very well with the expected 1:2:1 ratio. Single heads of the heterozygous plants of these three families were threshed and the percentage of hulled No. 624] INHERITANCE OF HULL-LESSNESS 23 kernels per plant determined as before. The three fam- iles gave the following distribution: Eta Bek Pore a Bee S Pye Er | ER Hi E E ad E + eh eee! grain on the heterozygous plant used as parent and the average percentage of hulled kernels on its heterozygous offspring. Dotted line represents the value for the parents and the solid line that of the heterozygous offspring. Series 202 From these distributions it is clear that the percentage of hulled kernels on the parent form influences the amount of the hulled condition. The average percentage of the offspring in each case agrees closely with that of the parent forms. | From these three families several plants differing in their percentage values were selected to continue the study in the fourth generation. As observed from the frequency distribution just given it is noted that series 7 is of high value, while 8 is relatively low and 9 varies from very low to high. The plants selected then in gen- 24 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIH eral represented the types of their lines; that is, those from 7 were generally high, those from 8 generally low, and those from 9 both low and high. The offspring of these various selections are arranged in a table similar to that for series ‘202. Per- Percentage of Hulled in Offspring aa Per cent- Cent. : ag z of Series No. Hulea) | alol elal elelelaleļe|ele|e| e| e| a| e| 2| 2| $ | nuna in| 2| o| $| S| X| ŠIS S| $| S/S] S| S| 2| S| 2| S| 2| S| 8| & | Ker- Pianta | $| $| 2! 2A eji 2] 3) 2/4) 4) 3) A) 3) 4) 3) 2) a) a) [nes on ajo) sel sisi gig! S| S| 3/8! $| 3| 5| £| £ 3/3/83] | apring 382al-7 | 73.3 1 1| 3/6 5| 5 6/12/10, 6) 1 55| 67.3 8} 37.7 |1|1;2)3]| 3; 641512; 5) 2) 1) 2 37| 33.7 9| 49.3 |1\/1/3/6}| 7 6|5|4| 4/14) 5| 811/11 7| 8| 1] 1] 2 105) 50.1 22 5 911129 / 9/7 |2212/14/18 17/1918| 7| 2| 2 TABLE VI SHOWING SEGREGATION IN F, OF CERTAIN F, PLANTS TOGETHER WITH THE PERCENTAGE OF HULLED SEED IN PARENT TYPE AND THE AVERAGE PERCENTAGE IN THE HETEROZYGOUS OFFSPRING Segregation Obtained from Plants Sown and Resulting Percentage of Hulled Kernels on Intermediate Forms. Per Per ulied Inter- | Hull Balled jase ana: | Por Genk: thutiea Kernels| Hulled} mediate} less Inter-| Hulled| Inter- and P. E. in Plan mediate mediate So Offspring $6241-7-10. .| 63.6) 22 25 63.1 22 56 28.21 + 3.3] 11.. 550 | 24 t 10 46.0 24 44 35.29 + 3.54 $2: 4 71.0 | 28 3 22 70.5 28 60 31.82 + 3.11 .33..| 70.3 21 3 26 62. 21 79 21.00 + 2.92 37.. 500 8 19 57.5 8 50 13.79. + 3.84 AS, | 817|- 18 7 13 75.1 13 40 24.53 + 4.( 55..| 68.0} 19 ) 28 67.8 19 67 22.09 + 3.15 8-14..] 43.6 | 26 L 19 37.6 26 53 32.91 + 3.29 17| 392| 28 3 23 4 28 86 24.56 + 2.74 Py Zee 16 } 14 25.3 15 50 | .23.08 + 3.62 Pele WE: 4 ) 9 19.2 4 18 18.18 + 6.23 Po 008] 12 30 13 49.8 12 43 21.82 + 3.94 35: | 179] 22 43 24 20.5 67 24.72 + 3.10 61..| 59.6 | 13 11 50.6 13 38 25.49 + 4.09 64. 9.8] 16 j 14 25.8 16 50 24.24 + 3.60 9- 9..; 14.0 4 ) 7 19.2 4 36 10.00 + 4.62 44 62.4 | 24 ) 15 49.1 24 65 26.97 + 3.10 04) ol 15 | 20 22.2 15 4l 26.79 + 3.90 166.. 797| 16 3 22 1.2 16 49 24.62 + 3.62 176. .| 879: 19 32 17 67.2 19 49 27.94 + 3.54 Tots. oc: 349 | 690 |351 349 1041 25.11 + .78 Expected | 847.5| 695 | 347.5 347.5 |1042.5 No. 624] INHERITANCE OF HULL-LESSNESS 25 The total number of plants in the hulled, intermediate and hull-less classes agree very closely with the expected numbers. The same is true for the 3:1 grouping, since the percentage of hulled is 25.11 + .78, which shows with- out doubt that the various families give offspring which follow the 1:2:1 expectancy. In this series there is little evidence that the percent- age condition of the parent plant affects the type of seg- regation in the following generation. In sini the segregation of the various families follows a 1:2:1 ratio regardless of the percentage condition of the parent. To show the relation between the hulled condition of the parent forms and that of the heterozygous offspring a correlation table was made in which the different fam- ilies were arranged according to their percentage values. The coefficient of correlation here is .726 + .012, which TABLE VII SHOWING THE RELATION BETWEEN THE PERCENTAGE OF HULLED KERNELS ON THE peen tex PARENT PLANTS AND THE PERCENTAGE OF ED KERNELS ON ITS OFFSPRING Percent- Percentage of Hulled Oats on Offspring Hulled elieiglaigigiziziaigigizizizieiaie|2 oatson|e|}S/2/2/8i8l/sieleisigisi¢isizielsigig¢is me S| Alaa aliaa Eaa PIS/SI8(R/8/8/S/3/S/8/S/8/F/2/28/18/8)/8 9.8 | 2| 2| 4| 5| 4] 5| 7| 2| 3] 2 36 14.0 | 2| 7| 7| 3| 1| 2| 2| 1| 2| 2 29 66 a 9 17.2 | AE 4| 5| 3| 1| 3 43 23.2 | 2| 2| 4| 5| 4| 7| 2| al 2 36 23.6 | 2 6j. 7| 2| 1) 1 1 20 38.2 2l 3/12 11| 11] 8| 7! 3| 1! 3 2| 63 43.6 2| | 5| 2| aļ 5| el e| 1| 2| a - 34 53.0 1) 1] 2} 1] 3} | 2] 2] 6 5} 63) 2 a) fa 34 59.6 1) | 4! ol 4! gi 4) ol al 4 27 60.8 1 2) 1) 3} 4) 3}: 6 2) 3) 3) 2 30 62.4 | 4| 2| 2| 1 1} 4| 2| 3] 5] 9| 6 4| 4] 1 2| | 50 63.0 | 1 2 1) 2} 5| 1} 6| 5| si | 1] af | 30 63.6 1 2} 1} 1} 2} 2| 2} 1] 9} 1] 3] 6 1 31 70.3 i] 1] 2} 2 | 7} 9] 7 Of S| 6 1 3 53 77.0 1 1 1) | 4| 1) 5] 9} 6 5] 3} a} | 37 79.7 Hini 2| 1| 4| al 1/10| 2| 27 80.0 | 1| 1 3 1} 3} 4 2) 3] 6 2} 3) a) a) fa) an 81.7 a) dat al bh al a) ai- 27 87.9 1 EE EE sl al a 31 17| 22| 29| 37) 31| 35/45) 41| 42| 46| 54| 49| 34| 57| 38| 40| 31| 17| 17| 5 | 687 26 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. LIII is considerably higher than it was with the 202 series. One reason for this may be that perhaps there is a dif- ference between this series and the former or that the result is caused by grouping the three families. When a correlation table is made for each of the three families ER se oe ee Se eee HS oe (ee ee SERS EEL : Hg Eg REH Senate ea i i n E E A D IHE HES egy KEN M HERS A ER RET RHH jii j A ii HEHA RE an i GE EEH a EA iH 8 i ETTAN HS ET EE À HAAN MAHR {ies UM Tes RP NY FAR ect A HEREN HMM Ge ea AN euhhh Me EEN i i EN if Hi! ANCE Nt i t BH l: iliii fiil eli i Uma GE E E A SN i Ra i [i SCHEER Mea l a AH REAA UG EF i e TRH E pa peg E a ‘ E E ilk ietan aT A À ee | fat EET Ha A RH i E i a en Ba a a a ANGH SIHI NIRA HHIH IAA RT EE E E e EN aie A A DE RNE RN ith tHE : X EEE He FR REN Fi; E E E a TR el Hab’ H p i EES a SG E a i a gg Tn ae 4 i] mi e oereerea i REEE EE] iba it Hil Be Bal i Aa di Bane nied FREY FD i EEH RA a Eh Alm aM E THEE EH HTD FASTEST ET H CEER EN RT f f} "E BRAR IA Hi £ H H ded HEH ib UEU LAH Pe ER AHO GNERRA INN ER EHTE ae E N AETV RUA RHH RR i= = E=] = am: ES IG. 7. These curves show the relation between the percentage of hulled grain on the heterozygous plant used as parent and the average percentage of hulled kernels on its heterozygous offspring. Dotted line represents the value for the parents and the solid line that of the hetrozygous offspring. Series 382. separately, correlation coefficients of .296 + .039, .623 + .025 and .741 + .024 are obtained. Thus it is seen that within any family correlation exists to a greater or less degree. Putting the three families in one table does in- crease the correlation somewhat over the average value for each alone. Another and possibly more plausible reason is the fact that these plants are of the fourth generation, while those of 202 are of the third. No. 624] INHERITANCE OF HULL-LESSNESS 27 The three parent series from which these were taken were of three types as mentioned before, therefore the parent plants selected from them carried the tendency to produce high or low as the case may be, and when they are all arranged in a correlation table naturally a high coefficient is obtained. In other words, the three parent forms were more nearly homozygous, so to speak, for high or low values. More will be said on this point later. This relationship was further shown by means of a graph showing the relation between the parent percent- age condition and the average value for the offspring, the same as was done in Fig. 7. In this case the relationship is higher than in the former series. The equatioa to the straight line is y = 17.2411 + 3.20622. That the plants arising from heterozygous plants hav- ing a low or high percentage of hulled kernels did not segregate in a manner indicating any influence of the hulled condition of the parent plant, as was the case to some extent with series 202, is shown in Table VIII. TABLE VIII i SEGREGATION OBTAINED WHEN SOWING SEED FROM HETEROZYGOUS PLANTS Havine Low oR HIGH PERCENTAGES OF HULLED SEED Percentage of Hulled Kernels Segregation of Offspring Into Different Types in Plants Sown ra ORINA eee sp 16 34 14 14.0 4 29 - 17.0 4 r : 23.2 15 14 23.6 15 21 76 f2 88 77.0 28 $3 er 79.7 16 27 22 sèn - 32 17 84 | 155 93 It is clear that the percentage of hulled seeds does not seem to influence the segregation as far as these data are concerned. 28 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vor. LII HULLED COMPARED WITH HULL-LESS KERNELS In order to learn whether there was any difference in the ratios produced by the hull-less kernels from the heterozygous plants the seed from the third generation plants used was separated and planted separately; that is, the hulled and hull-less from family 382a1-7—45 was planted separately so that the ratio may be determined on each lot of plants. This was done for all the families. These results are given in Table TABLE IX RESULTS OBTAINED FROM SOWING HULLED AND HULL-LESS iara FROM THE SAME HETEROZYGOUS INDIVIDUAL SEPARATEL esis Obtained from Segregation Obtained from Hulled Seeds Hull-less Seeds Family No. Hulled | Intermedi-| Hull-less Hulled Intermedi-| Hull-less Plants ate Plants| Plants Plants (ate Plants Plants 382al-7- 45...... 11 26 12 2 1 1 fog | E 18 22 20 4 9 5 F eo apne eae 19 27 10 5 t 0 Mas ates 5 18 28 az 10 10 5 Sess ae ee 20 39 21 1 14 5 7- 37 ae 8 25 19 0 6 0 FB 6 en% 14 31 25 5 8 3 STA 18 19 8 8 15 11 Be Hra 10 28 12 18 35 11 2a O 3 ra 4 12 29 10 8- 23.. ki 6 6 0 3 3 S- 26, ee 9 18 8 3 12 5 8+ BO, ceca 5 16 7 17 27 17 8- 61. 9 19 9 4 8 2 8- 64 3 7 + 13 29 10 Lede 3 6 3 1 23 4 9- 44...... 16 35 13 8 15 2 a ote 7 8 7 8 13 13 9-166...... 13 22 14 3 5 8 9-175 19 30 16 0 2 I ete cee 227 419 235 122 | 271 116 ; Ehana ah i AER aS 220.25! 440.5 220.25| 127.25 — 254.5 127.25 In many of the cases the numbers are too small to give good ratios, yet the important point is obtained from the summation of the two series. In each case these agree very closely with the expected numbers. If there was any difference we might expect the hulled kernels to pro- duce relatively more hulled plants and the hull-less rela- No. 624] INHERITANCE OF HULL-LESSNESS 29 tively more hull-less. The facts are the reverse. In the series from the hulled kernels the hull-less plants are in the majority and the opposite is true for the hulled plants from the hull-less seed. It is very evident that there is no relation between the kind of kernel (hulled or hull-less) sown from a heterozygous plant and the off- spring produced. GENERAL DISCUSSION From the foregoing data it seems without doubt that the inheritance of the hulled condition follows a simple Mendelian ratio giving in general 1 hulled, 2 inter- mediate, 1 hull-less. This is in accord with the results obtained by Norton, Gaines, Zinn and Surface, and others. In regard to the relation between the hulled condition of the heterozygous parent plants and of the offspring, it is clear that there is a very close agreement in regard to the hulled percentage. When high or low plants are selected they produce heterozygous offspring giving high or low percentage. In most cases, however, the usual 1:2:1 ratio is obtained. This is true in general in all cases of the 382 series but not so for 202. Whether the 202 series behaves differently or whether in reality it will agree with 382 will have to be determined with further work. The percentage relation shows that there is a variation from very low to a very high percentage. Owing to this fact and that any heterozygous plant tends to reproduce a simple monohybrid ratio, in which the heterozygous plants tend to follow the percentage relation, it seems at first that we are dealing with a case of multiple factors, in which one primary factor pair determines the hulled or hull-less condition and the other factors influence the hulled condition of those plants only that are hetero- zygous for the primary factors. This may be so, as the results of selecting high or low individuals seem to indi- cate. If, however, we assume a multiple factor series to 30 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII account for the facts, it is evident that, assuming all the factors involved to have equal value, we must have an F, type that is very nearly intermediate as regards its per- centage condition. This we have not observed in any of our series. The F, type, while being generally inter- mediate, is not so as regards its hulled condition, for it always contains fewer hulled kernels than hull-less. Thus, so far as the percentage relation is concerned, we do not have a strict intermediate. To be sure, there is a reduction of the multiple-flowered spikelet and other changes which cause the F, type to appear as an inter- mediate. With the usual multiple factor hypothesis assuming ordinary segregation, there must be a larger number of individuals ranging from 30 to 70 per cent. than we have at the extremes. With series 379 and 202 we do not have any indication of such a condition. On the other hand, there is a slight suggestion that series 382 does tend more nearly to a frequency distribution such as would usually be expected with the ordinary multiple factor hypothesis. When the third generation distribution of series 202 is observed (Correlation Table V) it is ap- parent that there is more of a tendency to pile up nearer the lower values. When the size of the classes is doubled a decided skew curve is obtained with the mode at class 0-9.9. As stated above, the seed sown to obtain the plants used in this distribution was selected from plants of high, low, or medium value, and this may influence to some extent the type of distribution. Yet, when one ex- amines the percentages of the plants used as parents, it is apparent that they are fairly evenly distributed. If, as suggested above, the nearly dominant primary factor pair influences the hulled or hull-less condition and the other factors influence the hulled condition of plants het- erozygous for the primary factor, then we would expect a piling up near the lower values. With series 382 there is a tendency for both the third and fourth generation percentage distributions to be No. 624] INHERITANCE OF HULL-LESSNESS 31 grouped around the middle classes. This is especially - true with regard to the fourth generation, especially when the size of the classes is doubled. The results of the different series are rather conflicting and it does not seem possible at pregent to explain all of them on a simple multiple factor hypothesis. It seems quite possible to explain series 382 on this basis (except the first generation) but the other types do not at present seem capable of such an explanation. The distribution in Table II, which is the third gen- eration of a cross between Danish Island and Hull-less, is skewed much the same as for the third generation of series 202. No doubt for these series there is some dis- turbing factor which causes such distributions and more data will be needed before a suitable explanation can be found to fit all of these cases. It may be that, since in crosses between two hulled sorts we have found some hull-less spikelets, we have combinations such that there is a tendency to produce an excess of hull-less kernels. This would influence the type of distribution consid- erably. At first one might assume that those individuals nearer the lower part of the distribution were like the F, types, however, from all the plants tested where the percentage of hulled kernels has been low the frequency distribution of the percentage of hulled kernels from the heterozygous plants has been low in general and has not ranged from very low to very high, as would be the case with seed from F, plants. These facts would help support the statement just made, which is to the effect that it is possible cer- tain crosses tend to produce an excess of hull-less kernels. SUMMARY From the results presented it is evident that hull-less- ness exhibits a simple Mendelian ratio of 1 hulled, 2 in- termediate, 1 hull-less. The intermediates show all gradations of hull-lessness from those nearly hulled to those nearly hull-less. -. 32 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LII The percentage of hulled kernels on the heterozygous plants seem, to indicate to some extent the percentage of - hulled kernels on the heterozygous offspring. No matter what percentage of hulled kernels is present on the heterozygous individual, it tends in general to produce a 1:2:1 ratio. The hulled and hull-less kernels from intermediate plants reproduce similar 1:2:1 ratios. ENVIRONMENTAL REACTIONS OF PHRYNOSOMA? A. 0. WEESE UNIVERSITY OF New MEXICO I. INTRODUCTION le General Distribution.—The horned lizards, more familiarly known as the ‘‘horned toads,’’ of the south- western portion of the United States and the northern states of Mexico form a very distinct group of the family Iguanide. Unlike most other comparatively large rep- tilian genera, this particular genus (Phrynosoma) is limited to a very special environment, and it is only in a region of relative aridity that these animals find a favorable habitat. Within the limits set by the above condition the specific habitats of the various species and varieties of the genus vary greatly, ranging all the way from the extreme aridity and great heat of Death Valley in southern California (Phrynosoma calidiarum Cope) to the comparative moisture and cold of the northern Rockies (Phrynosoma douglassti Bell and varieties). The species especially discussed in this paper are all found in the Southwest, under varying environmental conditions. Phrynosoma modestum, the specimens of which were taken near Albuquerque, New Mexico, close to the lower edge of the ‘‘mesa’’ or clinoplane region, at an altitude of about 1,700 meters, is distributed throughout New Mexico, and to a certain extent in the adjoining states, wherever conditions are similar to those in the above typical habitat. The rainfall here averages about 30 cm. annually, while the yearly evaporation from a free water surface is in the neighborhood of 200 em. The soil is 1Contribution from the Zoological Laboratory of the University of Tlinois, 33 34 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LII rather loose and friable, consisting principally of ‘‘Ti- jeras fine sandy loam’’ and containing, near the surface, a relatively large proportion of fine angular gravel and wind-blown sand. The color is a yellowish or yellowish brown. The vegetation is sparse, consisting of scattered grasses, Chrysothamnus, Gutierrezia, Salsola, Yucca, ete. This species is not found in the adjoining valley of the Rio Grande, nor in the mountains (Sandias) which border the ‘‘mesa’’ on the east (2,200 meters and above), where moister conditions prevail. In the mountains the rain- fall is probably twice as great, on the average, as on the ‘‘mesa,’’ although accurate data are not available, and the evaporation is much less, due to the lower temperatures which prevail. In the valley the water table is very near the surface of the soil (actual soil surface or above to 5 meters below the surface). Standing water is not found on the clinoplane except after very heavy rains, which sometimes fail for months. Phrynosoma douglassti ornatissimum, specimens of which were obtained with the above, has a much less re- stricted habitat, both locally and regionally. It is dis- tributed over a great deal of the eastern slope of the Rocky Mountains, even as far north as Canada, and, locally, extends into both of the regions described above as bordering on the clinoplane. It is, indeed, more abun- dant in either of these than in the clinoplane region be- tween, indicating that the determining factor in the dis- tribution’ in this case is similar in the lower valley and on the mountain side. As mentioned above, the aridity of these two regions is much less than that of the clino- plane. ‘The soil differences are also marked, in that the moister soils are more dense and contain more humus, derived from the more abundant vegetation. However, the variation in both regions is very great, from heavy clay to fine sand in the valley and from native rock to fine sand in the mountain. Phrynosoma cornutum does not occur in the same local area as that occupied by the species previously mentioned, No. 624] REACTIONS OF PHRYNOSOMA 35 although it also is of wide distribution. ‘This species is found throughout Texas and eastern and southern New Mexico, and has been reported from Nebraska, Arkansas, ete. In general, it appears to inhabit regions in which the mean summer temperature is slightly higher than that Pies A aaa me 4 er ` f Map showing the approximate geographical distribution of the species discussed in this paper. required by the other two species. The specimens here considered were obtained at Alamogordo, in the Otero Basin, New Mexico, where the mean temperature is higher by about 5° C. than at Albuquerque. 2. General Habits.—The general habits of the three species here considered are much the same, so no sepa- rate description will be attempted. The following dis- cussion will apply, perhaps, more accurately to Phryno- soma modestum than to either of the other species, but will, in general, be true of all. They are not, essentially, heat-loving animals, although tolerant of desert condi- 36 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LIH tions. They are found more abundantly during the earlier summer months, and during the autumnal rainy season, when the aerial temperature does not exceed 32° C. During these periods the animals move about actively all day, spending the night in protected nooks under vegetation, in the burrows of other animals, or buried beneath the surface of the soil. As the daily maximum temperature becomes greater they are to be found only in the early morning and in the later afternoon when the heat is less intense. During the heated part of the day the lizard is at rest, almost if not quite buried under the superficial layers of the soil. This position is reached in a characteristic manner. "The snout is directed down- ward and moved rapidly from side to side, the body ex- tremely flattened, while the legs take part in a rapid hori- zontally clawing movement. ‘The net result of this series of movements is to cover the animal with the loose soil, the depth varying according to the temperature, the char- acter of the soil, and other external conditions, as well as the individual. The same method of burrowing is em- ployed in preparation for hibernation, when the animal may bury itself under several inches of loose soil. In at- tempting to escape from enemies, other lizards have been observed to dig in a similar manner, and it is probable that Phrynosoma also escapes in this way. 3. Food Relations.—The food consists of various in- sects with which the animals come into contact, ants being more readily eaten by the smaller individuals and beetles (Eleodiini) forming a considerable portion of the diet of the larger ones. No food is taken unless it is living or at least moving. Sand grains set in motion by a heavy wind or otherwise are often snapped up, and sand grains are accordingly found in the feces. 4. Water Relations.—None of the species of Phryno- soma have been observed by the writer to drink water, and it is doubtful if water, independent of that contained in the insect food, is ever ingested. Many individuals are found in situations where there is never any standing No. 624] REACTIONS OF PHRYNOSOMA 37 water except after the very infrequent heavy rains. Very little water is excreted ordinarily, as when fed on ants, beetles, etc., the feces are eliminated as a dry mass con- taining practically no water, and the urine is composed of an equally dry mass largely made up of crystals of uric acid. When fed on a moist diet, such as grasshopper nymphs from a moist habitat, the feces become softer and are often accompanied by a considerable amount of muci- laginous liquid. The urine, however, remains as usual. The idea that the excretion of waste nitrogen as uric acid is an adaptation on the part of the Reptilia for life in arid regions is well borne out by the conditions in these animals. Urinary analyses made by the writer in the laboratory of physiological chemistry of the University of Illinois give the following results (1917b): onstituents Milligrams per gram SEOUL iron (i A ccc eee s cee 260.0 SIND MORON oe ns tac ees uote 1.4 Pred neiroren OLOA Ga a. 0.0 EET T T: MEREEN TD i aces ese Oe a So [Areonericat Piai Deia JA | : 3 THE UPPER i fl DRAINAGE AREA ir Fic. 1. Map showing the hypcthetical Permian drainage of the Upper Ten- nessee drainage area. Compare with the present drainage shown in Fig. 2. other animals, are exceedingly varied. All the factors which influence individual movements, as indicated in the first part of this paper, now apply in detail, and in addi- tion there is the pressure exerted by animals living asso- ciated with them. Simple animals require a favorable No. 624] MIGRATION A FACTOR IN EVOLUTION 69 environment as truly as complex ones. It is known that many animals decline in vigor if kept in the same medium, but if the medium is kept fresh, or the animal moves about freely and secures a fresh medium, it thrives. Freedom of movement thus permits the animal to move away from influences which interfere with its system, and thus minimizes the disturbance. The continuation of this process tends, with freedom, to bring the animal ul- timately into favorable non-interfering conditions if oS oF gs PRESENT DRAINAGE OF THE UPPER TENNESSEE RIVER SYSTEM _,J such exist. With these ideas in mind we need to recall Į that the atmospheric, hydrospheric, lithospheric and vegetational pressure all combine to encroach upon the animal, and to interefere or reinforce its activities and movements. The normal movements of the animal, and the ordinary routine of environmental changes, are thus in process of continual adjustment. Thus with the mi- gration of the animal habitat, whether caused by a change in the atmosphere, hydrosphere, lithosphere, vegetation, 70 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou LHI or any combination of these, the animals also tend to migrate with it and they are thus led about all over the surface of the earth. In this we see the importance of cycles of climatic changes, cycles of crustal movements of the earth, changes in circulation of the sea, and the suc- cession of plant and animal associations. It is to the changes of this character that the student of paleontolog- a gi Oe EA gr “oy - -3 meene iaa yl ins A e acme Siem woo maemo j A, “ Re i? TA y Bucs. Pe a < WR ae Æ mA ii YE K tt Jae a Oo Ke 5 ’ A 7 > PA N A 3 . K 4 N o we a oe ne phe Zs ueadvace KS : oe Fe È eor ta i D A ` { [is Louoow | i far Bi Vi y Vir A id pf Lys J ~ | Wenara Fie Map showing the hypothetical migrations of the snails of the genus Io in the upper Tennessee River system, as influenced by drainage changes. The numbers refer to the kind of shell illustrated in Fig. 5. ical evidences and causes of evolution gives much thought, and it is to the present evidences of these changes to which the field ecologist gives much attention. In my study of migrations of the fresh-water snail Jo, in the drainage of the upper Tennessee River system (Adams, ’15b), it was found that there were great cycles of change in the history of the streams, and that there were probably corresponding migrations of the snails. This is shown if we compare the map of ancient hypo- thetical drainage, Fig. 1, with that of the present, Fig. 2, and the supposed migration of the snails, Fig. 3, and compare these with the map of their present distribution, No. 624] MIGRATION A FACTOR IN EVOLUTION 71 Fig. 4. The shells of these snails are shown in Fig. 5. The presence of these snails in the headwaters of streams appears to be due to the ordinary creeping movements of the snails taken in connection with the up-stream migra- tion or growth of the stream habitat, because, on the other hand, the current tends only to favor a down-stream dispersal. Such animals, therefore, appear to be led about by the migration of their habitat. This sort of migration is comparable to those land migrations which Fra Fic. 4. Map showing the present distribution of the forms of the snail Io in the upper Tennessee River system. The numbers refer to the kind of shell illustrated in Fig. 5. have clearly taken place during climatic migrations, as during the ice age, and during similar changes in humid- ity, and with base-leveling ee (Woodworth, ’94; Adams, ’01). The competition among different kinds of animals has long been recognized as an important factor in animal migrations. Overcrowding produces a condition of stress, and as a result of this stimulus, animals tend to migrate and become diffused from the region of pressure in all possible directions. Thus new conditions are en- countered which necessitate changes on the part of the animal, and thus this process continues indefinitely. ou. LIL j [V NATURALIST 'AN THE AMERIC 2 SL UL poyRoIpU re suopeaigpu [LOTJeyJOdAY əsoqm ‘OF snuəg oy} UT SPYS JO swIogz ueu Əy} JO SUOPPLIJSNIJJI Q 'ÐIA as No.624] MIGRATION A FACTOR IN EVOLUTION 73 IV. Summary AND CONCLUSIONS The animal should be looked upon as a dynamic system which tends to continue in its course of action until changed from within or until diverted by external inter- ference with its system, and until a condition of relative equilibrium is developed by balancing all influences. The behavior of animals should be viewed as a process of rhythmical activity. The cycle of activity of the animal agent is a unit of _ fundamental importance. To study cycles, their dynamic status, their degree of relative equilibrium must be de- termined. In this manner the conditions of stress, the processes of adjustment to strain, and the conditions of relative equilibrium may be recognized and determined. These determinations should be applied to all cycles of activity, that of the life history, and all others. The use of these ideas enables one to apply Bancroft’s law—that a system tends to change to minimize external disturb- ance—to animal activities, and thus one is enabled to ex- plain a large number of diverse observations. Supple- mentary to Bancroft’s law are the influences which tend to accelerate or reinforce, without other change, the con- dition of the animal. The activities of animals cause them to collide with their environment. Conditions under which animals have become accustomed or attuned are those of relative equilibrium. With departure from these conditions, the animals are stimulated, their system is interfered with, and the animal tends to change until the interference is minimized. The hindrance thus placed upon animal activities are its ‘‘limiting factors,’’ and these are to be viewed according to Bancroft’s law. This law is not limited to the actions of the individual animal, but in- cludes also the race, and those of animal associations. The Vernon-DeVries law of the diminishing influence of the environment progressively during ontogeny, is an example of limiting factors according to Bancroft’s law. This law of Vernon’s is of great value in the study of 74 THE AZIERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII migration in relation to evolution because it suggests the critical period at which the stress of the new environ- ment may have its greatest direct influence upon the new generation and thus influence its heredity. The next important category above the animal system is the law of interacting systems. The main models of interacting systems are: . 1. The physical model of interacting forces, recalling in this connection the law of inertia, the tendency of a body to continue in its present state at rest, or in motion, and the law of reinforcement or acceleration. 2. Bancroft’s law is that a system tends to change in such manner as to minimize external disturbance. This should be applied to the interaction of all systems. This is a law concerned with responses to stress and to the process of adjustment, and it shows development or evo- lution of equilibria. 3. The phase rule applies to the result of responding to stress or equilibria. This is thus complementary to Bancroft’s law; one is concerned with the condition of stress, and the other with the condition of equilibria. These laws appear to be universal and not limited solely to the non-living. Irritability may not be causally explained, but it seems to obey these general laws in the same manner as causal changes. Applying these laws to animal migration, we see that the present geological age is one of physical stress, and that the process of adjust- ment to strain is now in operation. The physical stress applies to the air, water, earth and to their interactions. ‘This is an age of physical diversity—tending toward one of simplicity and uniformity. With diversity there are many local cycles of activity in all features of the en- vironment. These cycles of circulation influence the transportation of animals, and their active migrations. By transportation and migration animals encounter new conditions, new stresses, and change to minimize the dis- turbance and acclimate themselves to the limit of their No. 624] MIGRATION A FACTOR IN EVOLUTION 75 possibilities; and they repeat this cycle with unending monotony and persistence. December 15, 1917. V. BIBLIOGRAPHY Abbe, C. 1908. The Progress of Science as Illustrated by the Development of Meteorology. Smithsonian Report for 1907, pp. 287-309. Adams, C. C. 1901. Base-leveling and its (ore: Sie wera: with Illustrations from utheastern United Sta i ; . 839-85 1904. On the Analogy AE te Departure from Optimum Vital Arog and ay sips aad Geographie Life Centers. Science, N. S., Vol. 19, pp. 2 1908. The Took pteail Succession i bua The Auk, Vol. 25, pp. 109-153. 1909. Isle Royale as a as Environment. Ann. Rep. Mich. Geol. Surv. for 1908, pp. F 1913. Guide to the Study of pate Ecology. Pp. 1-183. New York. 1915. An Outline iat the Relations of Animals to their OnE Environ- ments. . Ill. St. Lab. Nat. Hist., Vol. 11, 1915a. An Ioann Study of ing me Toiset Sete sa Bull. Il. St. Lab. Nat. Hist., Vol. 19156. The Variations and tisotogtea? Shisha of the Snails of the Genus Io. Memoirs Nat. Acad. Sci., Vol. 12, Pt. 2, pp. 1-184. Bagehot, W. 1873. ef ne and Politics; or, Thoughts on the Application of the Principles of ‘‘Natural Selection’’ and ‘‘Inheritance’’ to Political Society. Pp. 1-224. New York. Bancroft, W. D. A Universal Law. Science, N. S., Vol. 33, pp. 159-179; hes Jour, Amer. Chem. Soc., Vol. 33, pp. 92-120, 191 Blackman, F. F. 1905. ans and Limiting Factors. Ann. of Bot., Vol. 19, pp. 281- 1908. ie Manifestations of Chemical Mechanics in the he Plant. ritish , 1908, pp. 1-18 (separate). Blackman, F, F., and Smith, A. M. 1 Experimental Researches on Vegetable Assimilation and Respira IX., On Assimilation in Submerged Water-Plants, Di ke Relation to the Concentration of Carbon Dioxide and other Factors. Proc. Royal Soc., B, Vol. 83, pp. 389—412, 1910. Brooks, W. K. 1902. The Intellectual Conditions for Embryological Science. Science, N. S., Vol. 15, pp. 481-492, Chamberlin, T. C. 1906. On a Possible Reversal of Deep-Sea Circulation and Its Influ- ence on Geologie Climates. Proc. Am. Phil. Soc., Vol. 45, pp. 1-11. 76 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vor. LIII wee T. C., and Salisbury, R. D. 4-06. Guotory. Vols. 1-3. New York, ona 6. M. 1915. A Dynamic Conception of the reggie Individual. Proc. Nat. daa tik pp. 164-172 915a. Senescence and Rejuvenescence. Pp. 1-481. Chicago. rag Individuality 3 in Organisms. Pp. 1-213. Chicago. Clements, F. C. 1916. Plant Succession, an Analysis of the Development of Vegetation. Carnegie Inst. of Wash. Pub. No. 242, pp. 1-512. Conant, C. A. 1908, The Influence of Friction in Economics. Science, N. S., Vol. 27, pp. 99-104 Cowles, H. C. 1911. T one of Vegetative Cycles. Bot. Gaz., Vol. 51, pp. 161- Davis, W. M. Gali ted by D. W. Johnson.) 1909. Geographical Essays. Pp. 1-777. New York. Fassig, O. 1899. Types of March Weather in the United States. Amer. Jour. Sci., (4), Vol. 8, pp. 319-340. (This paper, taken in conjunc- tion with others listed in this bibliography, will materially as- sist the zoologist in the applications of the process conception to the atmospheric problems with which he deals. Cf. Abbe, 708.) Findlay, A. 4. The Phase Rule and its Applications. Pp. 1-313. London. Henderson, L. J. 1 The Fitness of the Environment, An Inquiry zt the Biological Significance of Matter. Pp. 1-317. New Yor 1917. bes Order of Nature. An Essay. Pp. 1-234. Clan Mass. Hooker, Jr., 917. ae s Law of the Minimum in Relation to General Biological Problems. Science, N. S., Vol. 46, pp. 197-204 Huntington, B. 1915, Civilization and Climate. Pp. 1-333. New Haven, Conn. Jennings, H. 1906. Retavior of the Lower Organisms. Pp. 1-366. New York. 1912, Age, Death and Conjugation in the Light of Work on Lower Organisms. Pop. Sci. Mo., Vol. 80, pp. 563-577. 1913. The Effect of a aa in Paramecium. Jour. Exp. Zool., Vol. 14, pp. 279-391 Keyes, C. R. 1898. The Genetie Classification of Geological Phenomena. Jour. Geol., Vol. 6, pp. 809-815. pair sao ù 1917. À Quarter-Century of Growth in Plant Physiology. The Plant World, Vol. 20, pp. 1-15. Mellor, J. W. 1904. Chemical Statics and Dynamics, including the Theories of Chem- ical Change, Catalysis and Explosions. Pp. 1-528. London. No.624] MIGRATION A FACTOR IN EVOLUTION 77 Pierce, W. D. T916. A New Interpretation of the Relationships of Temperature and Humidity to Insect Development. Jour, Agr. Research, Vol. 5, pp. 1183-1191. Pike, F. H. 1917. The ond of Death. Jour. of Heredity, Vol. 8, pp. 195-199. Pike, F. H., Soa Seo 1915. The Peon ‘of Certain Internal Conditions of the ee in Organic Evolution. AMER. Nat., Vol. 49, pp. 321 TS , A. 1910. The Influence of Darwin on the Study of Animal Embryology. Pp. 171-184. Darwin and Modern Science. Cambridge. Shelford, V. E. 1911. Physiological Animal Geography. Jour. of Morph., Vol. 22, pp. 551-618. Thompson, D. W. 1917. On Growth and Form. Pp. 1-793. Cambridge. Thorndike, E. L 1911. Niiet Intelligence. Pp. 1-297. New York. Van Hise, C. R. 1904. The Problems of Geology. Jour. Geol., Vol. 12, pp. 589-616. Vernon, H. M 3. Vatiation in Animals and Plants. Pp. 1-415. London. Willis, B. 1911. What is Terra Firma?—A Review of Current Research in Isostacy. Smithsonian Report for 1910, pp. 391-406. Woods, F. A. 1910. Laws of pnyin Environmental Influence. Pop. Sci. Mo., , pp. 813- Woodworth, J. B. 1894. The Relation between tengeh and Organice Evolution. Amer. Geologist, Vol. 14, pp. 209-235 To one interested in some of the broader Dooa of dynamic ideas, the following additional papers will prove suggestiv: Hale, G. E. 1912-13. eed Other Worlds. The World’s Work, Vol. 25, pp. 66-182 (1912), 286-302 (1913). Special attention is called to the second part of this paper on the relation of gravitation to radiation as a cycle of transformation. Herrick, C. L. 1906. pipat of Dynamic Theory to Physiological Problems. J Comp. Neurol. and Psych., Vol. 16, pp. 362-375 Keyes, C. R. 1912. Deflatative Scheme of the Geographic Cycle in an Arid Climate. Bull. Geol. Soc. Amer., Vol. 23, pp. 537-562. Spencer, Herbert 1913-15. The Principles of w Vol. I, pp. 1-706 (1898); Vol. II, pp. 1-663 (1899), N. Y 78 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LII 1916. First ie pp. 1-550. New York. Sixth edition of 1900. Althoug Very, F. W. hr these books by Spencer are in many ways not biological problems which uses so many. ae cae dy- namic conceptions. After familiarity with more modern views, these books may be read with great profit, particularly his dis- win and Modern Science, 1910, pp. 450-455. 1902. A Cosmic Cycle. Amer. Jour. Sci., (4), Vol. 13, pp. 47-58; 97- 114; 185-196. 1913. Sas becomes of the Light of the Stars? Pop. Sci. Mo., Vol. 89-306. 2, pp. ERRATA P. 471, line 23 from top, third word should read o” P. 472, line 5, for ‘‘ontology,’’ read ‘‘ontogeny.’’ P. 482, line 29, for ‘‘really,’’ read tf readily.’’ SOME STUDIES IN BLOSSOM COLOR INHERI- TANCE IN TOBACCO, WITH SPECIAL REFER- - ENCE TO N. SYLVESTRIS AND N. TABACUM H. A. ALLARD U. S. DEPARTMENT OF AGRICULTURE Tae blossoms of varieties of Nicotiana ee exhibit three distinct colors, white, carmine and pink. In the writer’s crossing experiments, two vite. ered nicotianas were used, N. sylvestris, a species with long, slender, pure white blossoms, and a variety of N. tabacum from Honduras (S. P. I. No. 30887), with rather small, pure white blossoms of the tabacum type. The pink-flowered variety generally used was the Connecticut Broadleaf variety, although the varieties 70-leaf Cuban, a mammoth type of Cuban which appeared as a mutation in Connecticut in 1912,and Maryland Mammoth also were used. The carmine-blossomed tobacco? is a variety of tabacum sold by various seedsmen for ornamental pur- poses under the name giant red-flowering tobacco. This variety breeds true to blossom color and crosses readily with all the commercial varieties of tabacum. CROSSES OF PINK-FLOWERED VARIETIES WITH CARMINE- FLOWERED VARIETIES In the crosses Pink 9 X Carmine ¢ and their recip- rocals, Carmine, without exception, has been perfectly 1 The colors carmine and pink have been compared with Ridgway’s Color Standards and Color Nomenclature, 1912 edition. The carmine is practi- cally identical with Ridgway ’s carmine, shown on y 1. The pink matches almost exactly his Hellebore Red, shown on Plate 2 There seems to be little definite information à hand soneerning the origin of the carmine-flowered varieties of N. tabacum. O. Comes, in his monograph ‘‘ Delle Razze Dei Tabacechi,’’ Atti. Del R’Inst.d araa teekito di Napoli, Serie 6, 1905, pp. 77-306, speaks of the Nepal tobacco, a variety of N. tabacum, as having intensely red blossoms. The Rano variety is also said to have distinctly red blossoms, 79 80 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von LIIT dominant, so that all the blossoms of first generation plants bear carmine flowers. TABLE I FIRST GENERATION PLANTS OF CROSS CARMINE X PINK Year | Row Cross poe Remarks 1915 | 26A | Carmine 2 x Pink (Md. Mexpenot) Pi. ae 21 | Allcarmine 1916 | 21C | Pink (70 leaf Gaan G- X Mine esena: r ae is af 1916 | 26A | Carmine 9 X Pink (70 leaf ETS RST OTA m 1916 | 35A | Pink (Conn ea f) 9 X Carmine g. pnr z 1917 | 114A | Pink (Conn. Broadleaf) 9 X Carmine g. 50S EN Tot eA a in lee AN EASES Ea 9s ' 146 |All carmine TABLE II SECOND GENERATION PLANTS OF CROSS CARMINE X PINK Year | Row Cross Diente | Red | Pink 1915 | 27B | Carmine 9 X Pink (Md. Mammoth) g' 15 13 2 1916 | 19B | Pink (Conn. Broadleaf) 9 X Carmine à 39 30 9 19 ink (Conn. Broadleaf) 9 X Carmi 1 6 4 1917 | 91B | Pink (Conn. Broadleaf) 9 X Carmine g 42 29 13 1917 | 107C | Pink (Conn. Broadleaf) 2 X Carmi 9 4 1917 Pink (Conn. Broadleaf) 9 X Carmine F. 153 114 | 39 OGM re AS ED EEEN EE E ee eee 282 21i Ti From the data shown in Tables I and II, it is evident that the characters pink and carmine behave as typical unit characters, with carmine completely dominant. Segregation takes place in the 2d generation into carmine and pink blossomed plants very close to the theoretical ratio of 3 to 1. A heterozygous plant of the first generation of the cross Pink (Conn. Broadleaf) 9 Carmine ¢ was then crossed with homozygous carmine. Of 115 plants ob- tained in this cross, all were carmine in color, which is in accord with the expected result. Heterozygous plants of the first generation were now crossed with recessive pink with the following results. From these results it is evident that the theoretical ratio 1:1 which obtains in such a cross is very closely ap- proached. No. 624] BLOSSOM COLOR INHERITANCE IN TOBACCO 81 TABLE III CROSSES BETWEEN HETEROZYGOUS PLANTS OF THE First GENERATION OF THE CROSS (PINK 9 X CARMINE ĝ) AND PINK Year. | Row. Cross. sah Red | Pink 1915 | 18A | A first generation plant of the cross [Pink (Conn. Broadleaf) 9 X Carmine g] 9 X Pink (White Burley Mammoth) g ..... 19 T 12 1915 | 22 A first generation plant of the crozs [Pink (Conn. Broadleaf) 2 X Carmine o’] 9 ink ite Burley Mammoth) ~..... 41 17 24 1915 | 26B | A first generation plant of the cross [Pink erg aig cpio 2 X Carmine d] 2 X Pink (Md. Mammoth) o.............. 19 11 8 1915 | 28B ae generation "plane of the cross [Pink (Conn. Broadleaf Carmi Pink (White Burley Mammoth) g ..... 16 7 9 1917 first generation plant of the cross [Pink (Conn. Broadleaf) C ; Pink (White Burley Mammoth) g ..... 197 91 106 1917 A first generation plant of An cross [Pink C Broadleaf) 2 X Carmine g] 2? X ink (Conn. Broadleaf): Scorers 249 130 ' 119 TP E a a cos es Va ae oo 541 | 263 | 278 In further studies of the cross Carmine X Pink, the progenies of many extracted, recessive pink-flowered plants of the second generation have been grown, and all have produced pink-flowered lines. Of the carmine- flowered plants of the second generation, some (the heterozygous) have again broken up into Carmine and ink, while others (homozygous) have given pure Car- mine lines. Crosses Invotvine CARMINE oF PINK witH WHITE In crosses involving Carmine or Pink with White, white has behaved as a recessive, whether the cross has been made with the white-flowered species N. sylvestris, or with the white-flowered variety of N. tabacum S. P. I. No. 30887 from Honduras. Carmine, however, is not perfectly dominant in these crosses for plants of the first generation are somewhat lighter red than the car- mine itself. In the cross Pink X White (N. sylvestris) the blossoms of the first generation are somewhat paler than the usual pink of the tabacum varieties. In crosses 82 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LIII between pink-flowered varieties of N. tabacum and White (S. P. I. No. 30887 from Honduras) white has be- haved as a recessive. The intensity of the dominant pink depends upon the pink variety used. In some crosses, the blossoms of the first generation plants are similar in color to the pink of the pink parent. In other crosses, the pink of the first generation plants may be noticeably deeper than that of the pink-flowered parent. A number of heterozygous plants of the first genera- tion of the cross Pink (Conn. Broadleaf) 9 X Carmine g have been used as mother plants and crossed with the re- cessive white N. sylvestris, with the following results: 42 plants, of which 25 were carmine blossomed and 17 pink blossomed. 41 plants, of which 23 were carmine blossomed and 18 pink blossomed. Here we have a total of 83 plants, of which 48 possessed Carmine blossoms and 35 possessed Pink blossoms, show- ing an approximation to the 1:1 ratio. In crosses in- volving the species N. sylvestris, some difficulties are in- volved, since the first generation plants are usually sterile or nearly so. However, this sterility has been overcome in the cross in which a first generation plant of the cross (Pink (Conn. Broadleaf) 2 X Carmine ¢) was pollinated with the pollen of N. sylvestris. In the second generation of this cross, whites, pinks and carmines appeared. A number of carmine plants were selected and their prog- enies studied. One known as no. 12, proved to be homo- zygous for carmine and has bred true to this color for several generations. A sister plant no. 9 with carmine- blossoms proved to be heterozygous. In a progeny of 32 plants obtained from this plant, 26 were carmine and 6 were pink blossomed, approximating the theoretical ratio hep | In the cross Carmine X White, using the white-flowered variety of N. tabacum S. P. I. No. 30887 from Honduras, the plants produce an abundance of fertile seed. As has been stated, carmine is dominant over white, but it is not No. 624] BLOSSOM COLOR INHERITANCE IN TOBACCO 88 a perfect dominance as in the cross Carmine X Pink, for the blossoms of the first generation plants are somewhat paler than pure carmine. In the second generation there is a segregation into whites, and various degrees of pinks and reds, ranging from light pink to pure carmine. Of 278 second generation plants of this cross, grown in 1917, the blossom colors were grouped as follows: BOLO oss ey P Ga ee Pua se eee he ee cans 65 Dark pibk sy: Hie sie Motes CAS AA aba i 26 Laight pink iaat hg. Ces SL aN Ce es 38 Liphtor TOAD COIOMNOS so aaa No. 625] HYBRIDS AMONG SPECIES OF SPIROGYRA 115 munis by a, and the factor for the inflated sporiferous cell of varians by B and cylindrical sporiferous cell of communis by b, then the fusion nucleus in the zygospore would have the composition 4aBb. At the subsequent reduction division these four characters might be distrib- uted in four different ways: AB typical varians; Ab vari- ans dimensions and communis type of sporiferous cell; aB—communis dimensions and varians type of sporifer- AaBb. SPAS eleri Fig. 5. Diagram illustrating pi foiatta progeny of a cross between 8. varians d 8. communis. ous cell; and ab—typical communis. All four of shoe possibilities are represented by filaments in Collection 1877 from Campus Creek. Furthermore, the fertile cells of individual filaments and the zygospores formed are similar throughout a particular filament, as would be ex- pected on the basis of the origin of the final zygospore nucleus. This hypothesis, therefore, accounts for all the facts at present known concerning this series of collec- tions. The forms occurring in the two series of collections 116 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou LIIT from Lerna and Cossel Creek can be similarly accounted for. In this case, however, the two species, S. varians and ©. porticalis, evidently differ in three characters. The characteristics of varians have been given above. S. porticalis (Fig. 6) has vegetative cells 40-50» x 66-200 containing a single chromatophore, making three to four turns. The fertile cells are cylindrical and the zygospore is ovoid, 38-50» X 50-83». It, therefore, differs from S. varians in dimensions of the vegetative cells, in the absence of inflated fertile cells, and in the Fic. 6. Typical 8. porticalis showing single chromatophore, ovoid spores and cylindrical sporiferous cells. ovoid form of the zygospore. In both series of collections all of the eight possible combinations of these three char- acters occur. In the accompanying diagram. (Fig. 7) the dimension factor for varians and porticalis are represented by A and a, respectively. The factor for the varians type of fertile cell by B, and for the porticalis type by b. The factor for the ellipsoid spore by C and for the ovoid spore y c. The nucleus formed by the fusion of the two gamete nuclei therefore contains all the factors (Aa, Bb, Cc). The final zygospore nucleus contains any one of eight possible combinations: ABC, typical varians; AbC, varians dimensions and spore form, with porticalis fertile- cell form; Abc, varians dimensions, with portiealis fertile- cell form and spore form; ABc, varians dimensions and No. 625] HYBRIDS AMONG SPECIES OF SPIROGYRA 11T AbG aBe ABc aBC. doo a em Fic. 7. Diagram to illustrate hybrid progeny of a cross between S. varians and 8. porticalis. 118 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou LIII fertile-cell form, with porticalis spore form; abc, typical porticalis; aBc, porticalis dimensions and spore form, with varians fertile-cell form; abC, porticalis dimensions and fertile-cell form, with varians spore form; aBC, por- ticalis dimensions, with varians fertile-cell and spore rm. The female filaments derived from these several zygo- spores should show these eight possible combinations when they fruit. These forms have all been found in the two series of collections in each of two successive years, and there seems to be no question but what they are the products of hybridization and segregation. Since the in- heritance is entirely matriclinal, segregation occurs in the first generation. As to the relative numbers of the several types it is impossible to count filaments in a collection. Theoreti- eally they should be present in about equal numbers if all the filaments are of hybrid origin. In all the collections, however, there were filaments conjugating in the usual way, so that any attempt at counting filaments would be useless even though it were practically possible. SuMMARY 1. Hybridization between Spirogyra communis and S. varians and between S. varians and S. porticalis have been observed. The forms probably derived from these crosses have also been found. 2. Hybrids are exceedingly rare among species of Spirogyra. 3. They have been observed only in a few species. 4, Hybrid zygospores may be formed between species even though they have very different vegetative and spore characters. 5. The nuclei derived from the two gametes do not fuse until after the zygospore wall matures. The form of the zygospore is determined entirely by the female gamete. 6. The fusion nucleus of a hybrid zygospore contains factors for all the various characters of both species. No. 625] HYBRIDS AMONG SPECIES OF SPIROGYRA 119 When the reduction division takes place these factors are segregated in various combinations in the final spore nucleus. 7. When the filaments derived from hybrid zygospores fruit their hybrid character becomes evident in the dimen- sions of the filament, the character of the sporiferous cell and the form of the zygospores. 8. Inheritance is matriclinal and therefore the segrega- tion becomes evident in the first generation. LITERATURE CITED Bessey, C. E 1884. Hybridism å in Spirogyra. AMER. NAT., 18: 67. Wolle, Francis. 1887. Freshwater Alge of the United States. P. 218. Plate CXXXVIII, Figs. 5 and 6, and CXLII, Figs. 5 and 6. West and West. 1898. Ses hte age on the Conjugate. Annals of Botany, 12: 43; e V, Figs. 70 and 71. Andrews, F. = 1911. Conjugation between Two Different Species of Spirogyra. Bull. Torrey Botanical Club, 38: 299. Overton, E. 1888. Über bege eee e bei Spirogyra. Ber. d. Deutsch. Botan. Ges., 6: 68-72. Klehbahn, H. 1888, Uber die Zygosporen einiger Conjugaten. Ber. deutsch. Botan. Ges., 6: 160-166. Chmielewski, V. F. 1890. Materialen zur Morphologie und Physiologie des Sexualprozesses bei niederen Pflanzen. Arbeit. Ges. der Naturf. d. Charkower Univ., 25 Troéndle, A. 1907. “poe die — und Keimung von Spirogyra. Botan. Zeitung, 65: 1911. Uber ye Hoduktonsteilung in den Zygoten von Spirogyra und über die Bedeutung der Synapsis. Zeitschrift fiir Botanik, 3: 593-619. Karsten, G. 1908. Die a ae der tek von aes jugalis Ktzg. Flora, 99: Kurssanow 1911, ibe Befruchtung, Reifung, und Keimung bei Zygnema. Flora, 104: 65-84. SYNTHETIC PINK-EYED SELF WHITE GUINEA-PIGS! DR. HEMAN L. IBSEN INTRODUCTION In the fall of 1914 Professor W. E. Castle kindly sent to the Department of Experimental Breeding, Univer- sity of Wisconsin, five guinea-pigs representing some of the rarer color varieties, suggesting that their genetic make-up be studied independently. These animals were subsequently turned over to the writer for investigation. During the winter two of them died without offspring and hence only three were left the following spring. Two of these proved to be what has been called by Castle red-eyed agoutis and the third was a ‘‘ pink-and-red- eyed’’ tortoise. The factors involved in the production of these animals will be described below in more detail. Castle (1914) had already described the different fac- tors found in these animals and had given some indica- tion of their relationships to other factors. At this time he stated that red-eyed was allelomorphic to al- binism, and that pink-eyed was recessive to the usual dark-eyed condition. In 1915 Wright showed that red- eyed formed an allelomorphic series with albinism and dilute and intense pigmentation. This made the account of the relationships of the factors fairly complete. At about this time it occurred to the writer that with the proper combination of the newly described factors and other factors it should be possible to produce an ani- mal which to all appearances would be an ordinary al- bino, but entirely different genetically from what have hitherto been known as albinos. Matings were imme- 1 Papers from the Department of Geneties, Wisconsin Agricultural Ex- periment Station, No. 13. Published with the approval of the Director of the Station. 120 . No. 625] PINK-EYED SELF WHITE GUINEA-PIGS 121 diately started with this in view, but since there were only three animals to work with at the start progress was necessarily slow at first. In the meantime Detlefsen (1916) had described some pink-eyed white mice carrying the color factor. He be- lieved they were due to the combination of the dominant self white condition and the pink-eyed, but in his paper this was not fully demonstrated. Castle (1916) refers to two pink-eyed white guinea- pigs, presumably albinos, which were born to pigmented parents. The male parent was tested with true albinos, but all of the eleven offspring obtained were pigmented. Castle therefore supposes that the two pink-eyed white offspring were not true albinos, but ‘‘ pink-and-red-eyed”’ animals lacking the factor for the extension of black or chocolate pigment. This is undoubtedly the correct ex- planation, as the evidence presented in this paper will demonstrate. Castle, however, must have discarded his pink-eyed whites before he realized their importance since he makes no mention of testing them genetically. Before going into further detail it may be well to de- scribe briefly the various factors directly concerned in the production of the pink-eyed self whites. Some of those indirectly concerned are also described in order to give the proper orientation. DESCRIPTION OF FACTORS B, the factor for black pigmentation. When unmodi- fied by other factors black pigment is found only in the eyelids, mammae, external genitals and the .skin of the ears and feet. b, the factor for chocolate (or brown) pigmentation. E, the factor for extension of black or chocolate throughout the pigmented part of the coat. The unpigmented parts are of course white. e, the partial extension factor. This factor causes black or chocolate to be only partially extended and therefore to appear in blotches, the remainder Zii8.i 991866.2 £1830.) Litter 1514 1202.4 2171.3 No. 625] PINK-EYED SELF WHITE GUINEA-PIGS 123 of the pigmented part being red. Guinea-pigs spotted black and red in this manner are commonly known as tortoises. e, the non-extension of black or chocolate. In this case black or chocolate are found only in the places men- tioned under B, 7. e., in the eyelids, ete. E, e an e form an allelomorphic series (Ibsen, 1916). They are given in the order of their dominance. C, intense pigmentation. Ca, dilute pigmentation. Yellow is dilute red. Cr, red-eyed or non-yellow. In a C, animal red (or yellow) is never present in the coat and black or chocolate are somewhat dilute. Owing to the ab- sence of red the amount of pigment in the eyes is decreased, thereby partially allowing the blood to show through. It is because of this reddish tint to the eyes that they have been called red-eyed by Wright (1915). However, it seems more proper to call them non-yellows since the absence of yellow in the.coat is the more general and striking effect produced by the factor. A C+ tortoise is there- fore black-and-white because the red spots of the ordinary tortoise are here absent. An eC animal is a self red, but an animal with the composition eC; is a self white, owing to the absence of red pig- ment. Such an animal will be referred to in the text as a ‘‘non-extended (e) non-yellow (C,).”’ From left to right: 911181, a gah gs non-extended non-yellow ; esai an albino carrying both black (B) he extension factor (E); g 1830.1, an na to the pink color of the skin of the ears of Ọ 1118.1 agy g 1830.1, and also be- ause of the a of the ai causing them cast shadows, the ears appear darker than they really are. The pink eyes eleye appear dark. Fig. 2. Gu seer g 849.1 is a bb dark-eyed non-extended non-yellow, ¢ 1258.1 isa a non-extended non-yellow carrying black (B). Fig. 3. Guinea pigs 4 1202.1, a pink-eyed mon-extended non-yellow (ppC,0,ee), 9 1171.38, an ais heterozygous for partial extension (ep), and three of their aoe litter 1514. Fig. 4. A nearer and clearer view of g 1202.1 and Q 1171.8, already shown in Fig, 3. Fie. 5. Guinea-pigs 1202.1 and 9 1118.1, both pink-eyed non-extended n: yellows ag one of their 24 offspring, § 1587.1, also a pink-eyed Demei non-yello 124 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vor. LIII The C, factor has been described in some detail be- cause it plays an important part in the production of the synthetic pink-eyed self whites. C and Ca are dominant to C+, while it in turn is dominant to Ca: Ca, albinism. Albino guinea-pigs generally tend to have some pigmentation on the nose, ears and feet. This varies in amount depending on the-other fac- tors present. A be albino is almost if not quite devoid of pigment (& 1830.1, Fig. 1), while a BE ‘albino is heavily pigmented at its extremities (2 1866.2, Fig. 1). In all cases, however, the eyes are pink and the greater part of the coat is white. P, dark-eyed. p, pink-eyed. The eyes are as pink as those found in albinos. Black (B) or chocolate (b) are also af- fected, becoming quite dilute, while red is unaf- fected. It will be seen from the description of the factors given above that if we can obtain an animal which is a non-extended non-yellow, eC,, and therefore a self white, and which in addition is pp, or pink-eyed, we shall have a pink-eyed self white (eC,p), which is not an albino in the ordinary sense of the word as applied to guinea-pigs, but which nevertheless to all appearances in an albino (see 91118.1, Fig. 1). There is one difference, however. Albinos vary considerably in the amount of pigment in the ears, nose and feet depending on whether E, e°, or B are present or absent. The synthetic pink-eyed self whites on the other hand cannot carry E or e, but may carry B. The pink-eyed factor (p) dilutes black pig- ment (B) to such an extent that it is impossible to tell by the appearance of the animal whether or not black is present. All the synthetic pink-eyed self whites are therefore a pure white and fulfil the guinea-pig fancier’s requirements for a good albino better than most true albinos do. No. 625] PINK-EYED SELF WHITE GUINEA-PIGS 125 BREEDING OPERATIONS As previously stated there were three animals with which to begin breeding operations in the production of the synthetic pink-eyed self whites. These were two non-yellow agoutis, 360.1 and $361.1, and a pink-eyed non-yellow tortoise, 2363.1. By various test matings these were proven to be of the following gametic com- positions: $360.1, AaPpC,C,e’e’,? 2361.1, AaPPC,C,ere?’, and 2363.1, aappC,Cre’e’. It will be seen that all three were homozygous for e”, the factor for the partial extension of black or chocolate. On this account it would be impossible by mating the animals together to produce the pink-eyed self whites since the desired animals must lack the extension factor and therefore be ee. What seemed the best method of procedure was to mate $363.1 (ppC,Cre’e’) to a self red (PPCCee), and then to inbreed the F,’s (PpCC;e’e). These all looked like ordinary tortoises. Since this was a tri-hybrid cross and since the desired animals were to be homo- zygous recessives, it would theoretically be necessary to have 64 offspring for the production of one of the desired type. This proved to be a very slow process and was finally discarded in favor of other less methodical mat- ings, which were more rapid in their results. The most successful will be described in some detail. A heterozygous tortoise $572.1, of the composition PpCC,e’e obtained by mating ¢ 360.1 to a self red, was mated to a yellow-and-white female, 629.3, carrying al- binism (PPC.C.ee). One of their offspring, $849.1 (Fig. 2), was at first mistaken for an albino, but more careful examination proved him to be a dark-eyed non- extended non-yellow. The reason for mistaking him for an albino was that he did not carry black (6).* His 2 A is the factor for agouti. 3 In a dark-eyed non-extended non-yellow (PeC;) the presence or absence of black (B) is as easily detected as it is in an albino or a self red. In Fig. 2 gf 1258.1 carries black while ¢ 849.1 does not. In addition to having much lighter colored extremities a bb non-extended non-yellow also has much pinker eyes than one which carries black. For this reason it may be mis- taken for an albino. 126 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von LIH gametic composition turned out to be PpC,Caee. He was mated to a pink-eyed tortoise, 2? 734.2,4 whose com- position judging by her offspring, must have been ppCaCrere. This cross may be represented as follows: g 849.1 x 2 734.2 PpC,Cgee ppCaCrere E re plae Gametes ae ne pUre pCae pCre Offspring Obtained PpCaC eve r PpCaCaePe | Dark Syou tortoise i Me oa li aa 1 ppCaC ere C : ppCaCaere } Pink prea? (OLLO = os aw aar aa A es 2 PpCaCree PpCaCa } Dark-eved solt yellow oe S a Cle WN oe es 2 ppCaCree ie i PP }Pink Protal- yelow oe int Vanes Goh ck EER 2 s PpC,Cr : PpC;-Caeve | Dark-eyed non-yellow tortoise .......:........... 2 ppC,C,ere i 4 pp0;0a } Pink eyed non-yellow tortoise .................. 0 PpC,C,ee - - SVOHOW ar ei es 1 PpC,Caee | Dark eyed non-extended non-yellow ppCrCree i r 2 ppC,Caee | Pint-eyea non-eatended non-yellow ........--.406 PORE oS ie a OO on a A ek 12 It will be seen that according to expectation there should be equal numbers of 8 different phenotypes. The 12 offspring actually obtained are remarkably close to expectation. At the time: ihia cross was made the gametic composi- tion of the mother, 2734.2, was not definitely known. When, therefore, the two pink-eyed non-extended non- yellows were born one could not be certain that they were not just ordinary albinos. The only definite method 4 Ancestry of 9 734.2: 8 179.1, PPCCaee Q 734.2, ppCaC ere 2 363.1, ppC,C,erer |9 549.3, PpCrCrever S sere? pee oe g 346.1, PpCaC;ere No. 625] PINK-EYED SELF WHITE GUINEA-PIGS 127 to test this was to mate them to true albinos, and this was accordingly done. One of these animals, 21118.1 (Figs. 1 and 5), mated to an albino, $596.3 (PPC.Cu.ee), had 4 dark-eyed non- extended non-yellow offspring and 2 albinos. This would make her composition ppC,Ccee and the cross may be represented as follows: g 596.3 x © Q181 PPC,Cagee ppCrCaee pC re Gametes: 3 PC,e Offspring: PpC,Caee, dark-eyed non-extended non-yellow; PpCaCaee, albino. The other animal, (1202.1 (Figs. 3, 4 and 5), was mated to an albino (Ẹ 1171.3, Figs. 3 and 4) of the cvm- position PPC.C.e’e. There were 12 offspring. Of these 6 were dark-eyed non-yellow tortoises and the other 6 were dark-eyed non-extended non-yellows (see litter 1514, Fig. 3). This would indicate that ¢1202.1 was of the composition ppC,C,ee, and the cross would be as follows: 1202.1 x ọ 1171.3 ppCrOree PPCaCae?e Gametes: $ pore ey Offspring: PpC,C,ere, dark-eyed non-yellow tortoise ;6 PpC,C,ee, dark-eyed non-extended non-yellow. It will thus be seen that the two pink-eyed non-ex- tended non-yellows were not of the same composition, the male, 1202.1 being homozygous for C,, while the female, 1118.1, was CCa. This is what one might expect by reason of their parentage. According to expectation, these pink-eyed non-ex- tended non-yellows when bred together should have 5 The albinos used in these test matings were unquestionably homozygous dark-eyed (PP) since pead came from stock that has never been known to T w pink-eyed factor (p) e dark-eyed non-yellow tortoises as sa as the dark-eyed non-ex- rs non-yellows are being inbred, and the results from these matings will be reported at some future date. 128 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Von. LII nothing but pink-eyed non-extended non-yellow offspring, Thus: 4 1202.1 x Q 1118.1 ; ppCrCree ppCrCaee af pCve Gametes: 1 pcre a pe pte Offspring: 4 if ccxeet pink-eyed non-extended non-yellows. The mating has been made and thus far there have been 24 offspring, all of them pink-eyed non-extended non- yellows (see Fig. 4). Some of these offspring are being tested by being mated to albinos, but as yet their compo- sition with respect to the presence of albinism is not definitely known. It may be of interest to mention that one of them when mated to a PPEE albino had one dark- eyed self black offspring. The other two offspring in the same litter were black-and-white, due to the fact that neither parent was homozygous for entire pigmentation Discussion In most domesticated mammals an albino, as ordi- narily understood by breeders, is a completely self white animal with pink eyes. Albino rabbits are of this type. In addition there is the Himalayan variety which also has pink eyes, but the coat instead of being entirely white is pigmented at the animal’s extremities. This condi- tion is dominant to albinism and recessive to the fully pigmented condition, thus forming part of an allelo- morphic series (Sturtevant, 1913). Guinea-pig fanciers also have what they call a Himalayan variety. Here, however, the genetic relationship differs from that found in rabbits. Himalayan guinea-pigs are undoubtedly true albinos carrying the factor for black (B) and the exten- 7 An § animal is entirely pigmented, while one that is ss shows some white spotting and is therefore not entirely pigmented. In this connection it may be of interest to note that in a non-yellow tortoise the white spotting may be due to two different causes, (1) because of the non-yellow factor (Cp) the yellow of the ordinary tortoise is here white, and (2) if the animal is ss it will show some white spotting on this account also, No. 625] PINK-EYED SELF WHITE GUINEA-PIGS 129 sion factor (Æ) which extends the black. The presence of these two factors tends to make the extremities heay- ily pigmented. The albino guinea-pig, according to the fanciers’ standard, should be as completely white as the albinos of other species. This, however, has never been entirely attained, even though much selection has been practised. Albinos most nearly approaching the standard lack both black and extension factor and are therefore eb. When mature they ordinarily show, nevertheless, a lightly pig- mented rim along the edges of the ears. The synthetic pink-eyed whites, produced as explained in the earlier part of this paper, are, on the other hand, pure white, and they therefore satisfy the fanciers’ standard in this respect. They would meet his desires, furthermore, in that they breed true for this character. There are other means besides those already men- tioned whereby pink-eyed self white guinea-pigs could be produced which would satisfy the fancier’s standard. One method would be to combine a self white condition described by Castle (1905) with the pink-eyed. Self whites of this type, however, do not breed true. Very frequently they throw spotted offspring. They seem to be merely an extreme form of white spotting, all of the animal being unpigmented except the eyes. Pink-eyed self-whites of this type would necessarily on this ac- count be very unstable in the transmission of their coat character. Another method would be to prodyce a ‘‘ pink-eyed ”’ (pp) albino. The pink-eyed factor in this case would cause the pigmentation ordinarily found in albinos to become invisible. We have produced an animal of this type. When mated to pink-eyed it had nothing but pink- eyed offspring, and when mated to albinos it had only albino offspring. It was pure white in color. It seems quite probable that synthetic pink-eyed self whites may also be produced in rats. Castle (1914) has already described yellow varieties and pink-eyed varie- 130 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LIII ties, and Whiting (1916) has stated that the non-yellow factor has been found in some animals. In conclusion it may be said that we have furnished one more proof of the fact that the phenotypic appear- ance of an animal may entirely mislead one as to its gametic composition. The synthetic pink-eyed self white guinea-pig may also serve another purpose. By proper matings an animal can be produced which carries all the known recessive color factors in guinea-pigs except al- binism, and animals of this type should be most useful in determining the possible linkage relations between the factors. LITERATURE CITED Castle, W. E. ‘ 1905. Heredity of coat characters in guinea-pigs and rabbits. Carnegie Institution of Washington, Publication No. 23, 78 pp. 5 pls. 1914. Some new varieties of rats and guinea-pigs and their relation to oblems of color inheritance. AMER. NAT., Vol. 48, pp. 65-73. Castle, W. E., ghy Sewall Wright. 1916. Studies of inheritance in guinea-pigs and rats. Carnegie Insti- tution of Washington, Publication No. 241, 192 pp. Detlefsen, J. P 1916. Pink-eyed white — carrying the color factor. AMER. NAT., Vol. 50, pp. 46-4! Ibsen, H. L 1916. Tricolor inheritance. I. The tricolor series in guinea-pigs. etics, Vol. 1, pp. 287-309. Sturtevant, A. H 1913. The Windleyss rabbit case with some eT ee on es allelomorphs. AMER. Nat., Vol. 47, pp. 23 Whiting, P. W. 1916. A new color variety of the Norway rat. Science, N. S., Vol. 43, p- 781, PARTHENOGENESIS AND CROSSING-OVER IN THE GROUSE LOCUST APOTETTIX! PROFESSOR ROBERT K. NABOURS Kansas STATE AGRICULTURAL COLLEGE AND EXPERIMENT STATION INTRODUCTION Tere have been found among the grouse locusts, genus Paratettix, in nature, fourteen factors for color pat- terns, all in one series of multiple allelomorphs. A fif- teenth factor, a modified S, in the same series, has orig- inated in the laboratory by means as yet not understood. Another factor, 9, for melanism, discovered in nature, has been found to segregate independently of the multiple allelomorph group (Nabours, 714, °17). Still another factor, $, for red-all-over, yet to be described, also from nature, behaving precisely as does @, though segregating independently of both it and the multiple allelomorph series, has been bred in considerable numbers. In one species of the genus Tettigidea,’ bred in our laboratory, there have been described a series of five multiple allelo- morphic factors for patterns, and an independently seg- regating color factor of the behavior of 4, or ¢, in Para- tettix (Bellamy, 717). In these experiments, involving several hundred kinds of matings and many thousands of individuals, with only the two exceptions, both as yet unexplained, segregation has taken place as expected. Among another genus, A potettiz,* of the grouse locusts 1 Paper 25 from the "gaia Laboratory, Kansas Agricultural College and Experiment Statio 2 Paratettix texanus Hancock Kindly identified ee both Dr. J. a Han- cock and Mr. Jas. A. G. R 3 Tettigidea parvipennis allie Hancock. 4 Identified by Mr. Rehn as Apotettix eurycephalus Hancock, and by Doctor Hancock as follows: ‘‘nearer the Mexican species Apotettix con- verus Morse, than the nearly allied Texan species, Apotettix eurycephalus Hancock. Inasmuch as you have used material from both Texas and Mexico in your experiments, it is possible you have hybridized the two.’’ The nat- ural history of this group has been described (Hancock ’02). 131 132 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LII there have been discovered in nature eleven factors for color patterns, all in the same series, but evidently only a few, if any, are allelomorphs. Pending further con- sideration, the patterns are designated as AA, GG, KK, MM, OO, RR, TT, WW, XX, YY and ZZ, respectively (all conspicuous, except AA which is of a mottled gray ground color and well protected). These patterns are as sharply defined and distinct, each from any other, as are those of Paratettix. Any two make a readily recog- nizable hybrid pattern, with the elements of each parent pattern seemingly equally represented, except that the part of a hybrid pattern produced by the factor A, when it is a member, is less clearly perceived, and such hybrids can not be, in every case, superficially distinguished from the pure, or homozygous, pattern of the more apparent member. For instance, it requires trained and careful scrutiny to distinguish between AK and KK, AW and WW, AY and YY, etc. It appears that the pattern AA, if it be the result of only the one factor, is quite different from the others, not only in its manifestation in the hybrid of which it may be a member, but also, as will be shown later, in that it appears to result from crossing- over among the others. On the other hand, KW, KY, WY, and most other hybrid patterns not containing A as a member are sharply distinct, each making a composite picture of both components. Even in ease of linkage, where three, four or more factors are combined, the in- dividual presents the composite appearance of all the patterns involved. For example, in KMR, KMR or KRM, KYZ and KRYZ individuals all the patterns con- cerned are clearly visible and apparently equally repre- sented. In respect to representation of patterns in the hybrids, as well as in the actual resemblance of a few of the patterns, there is striking parallelism between some of the members of this genus and some of those of Paratettix. However, as already suggested, in contrast with the inheritance behavior in Paratettix, most of the factors No. 625] PARTHENOGENESIS AND CROSSING-OVER 133 in A potettix show from small to considerable percentages of crossing-over. The discovery of crossing-over, with the further disclosure that these forms breed partheno- genetically, as well as bisexually, prompts me to submit a preliminary report in advance of a more extended presentation of the data and illustrations which can not be made ready this year. The Adams fund has cared for the expenses, and I have had the generous and open-minded support of di- rector, now president, W. M. Jardine. Mr. A. W. Bel- lamy gave effectual assistance during the earlier stages of the experiment. PARTHENOGENESIS AND CROSSING-OVER IN THE FEMALE The discovery of parthenogenesis in Apotettix was the result of attempts at cross-breeding the members of this genus with those of the genus Paratettix. It was observed that when an Apotettix male was used with a Paratettix female no progeny ever resulted, but the female of the reciprocal gave offspring, exclusively females, and of her own color pattern if she were homo- zygous, or segregated into her components and cross- overs if she were heterozygous. Then it was soon ascer- tained (see chart) that the Apotettix females which had never been exposed to males of any kind at any time be- haved in this respect precisely as did those exposed to Paratettix males. Copulation between members of the two genera was never observed. Confirming these ob- servations, Dr. J. L. Hancock kindly examined speci- mens for me and concluded that on account of structural differences the members of the one group could not mate with those of the other. The chart illustrates a portion of the experiment which definitely determined that parthenogenesis occurs. It also shows crossing-over in the females. Following the chart, the first KK female, exposed to a Paratettix male of strongly contrasting pattern, produced 10 offspring, all females and of her own pattern. Three of these F, 134 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII Paratettix x KK pole o-i E T Ptx x KK Ptx x kK Ptx x KB i te air Same9 0-7 0-10 OIA | : | | EKK l SKK | | KK KK? RR x KK TT x KK Ptx x KK=0-7 Ptxx KK=0-4 RY x KK Ptx x KK=0-1 on tke | | tx x KK=0-8 Ptx x KK=0-49 Ptx x KK=0-2 KK KR KT KK Ptx x KK=0-12 Ptx x KK=0-4 ER KY Q Ptx x KK=0-8 0-1 49-39 10-4 0-15 0-27 Ptx x KK=0-7 7-12 9-13 0-32 Ptx x KK=0-9 a w w UH A > 2 =0-68 ST Ty ‘Se Ptx x KK=0-22 88 14 15 ET L x] Re=0-41 à x KE=0-22 0-173 0-64 Y 173 ‘ 64 Same? a. Te = EM x KK KK9? KK9? KY99 KK9¢ sf Sete DENEN Pen: EA P RUR | aA + Ky kk kk kM a: ee E oe 20-28 Q-10 7-6 13-19 0-40 Q-163 0-1 0-1 0-2 0-0 0- 48 10 15 25 3-14 165 9-0 0-11 0-8 0- 4. as Q3 Q12 Q5 = Q-59 2 2B e individuals were separately exposed to Paratettix males and gave 7, 10 and 14, respectively, and again all were KK females like the female parents and grandparent. One of these F, females was subsequently mated to a male of the strongly contrasting pattern MM, of her own genus, and then she produced 12 males and 16 females of the composite pattern KM. From left to right, in F,, a female KK was mated to an RR male and gave, in F}, 4955:3999 of the inter- mediate KR pattern. About three weeks after the death of the male this KK female was removed to another cage, where she produced a few offspring, the only one re- corded being a KK female. The next F, KK female was mated to a TT male of a strongly contrasting pattern. She produced 1044:499 of the hybrid pattern KT, and 15 KK’s, all females and without a trace of the TT pat- tern. The following three F, KK females were placed with Paratettix males and produced 7, 8 and 12 KK females, respectively. The next four F, KK females were also exposed to Paratettix males, and gave 4, 49, 4 and 7 offspring, all like the female parents; while the re- maining two sisters, without exposure to males of any kind at any time, gave, in F, 68 and 41 KK females, respectively. : One of these, the one having produced 68 offspring No. 625] PARTHENOGENESIS AND CROSSING-OVER 135 Same 9 pe pe TEN KM 12-16 F: MT x KK OY x KK RR x Ptx x KM=0-0-0 0-2 0-5 0-0 aee ieoi Ee ee : x x KM=0-0- 0-0 Q-4 11-21 13-26 13-4 9-17 0-0 —x 2KM=0-0-1 0-15 0-18 0-1 4 32 39 14-22 12-18 1-1 0-0-} 0-26 0-36 0=] sik tai 27-26 21-35 1-1 26 Z8 53 56 2 Explanation. KK? MM x KK YY x KK Ptx=Paratettix male. Males on the left; aE a ass aR females on the right be a 27° KK KM Ky KK females. O0-0O-1=Sex not dete 2-2 0-3 0-1 O-3 Fa 49-39-49 males:39 females. All other eer r ae ee adipi refer to factors in Apotettix anā pattern parthenogenetically, was mated to a YY male and gave KY 2044:2899 and 10 KK 22 without a trace of the YY pattern. The other F, KK female, which had given 41 offspring parthenogenetically, when mated to a KM male, gave KK 745:6992, and KM 1355:1099. It is obviously impossible to determine if all the KK off- spring from this mating were produced bisexually, or some of them parthenogenetically. However, since par- thenogenetically produced individuals are, with rare ex- ceptions, females, the result is somewhat checked by the proportion of 75:699. From the 68 F, offspring sev- eral females were allowed to reproduce without exposure to males of any kind at any time, and 163 KK females resulted in F,. From the 41 KK F, individuals one which had never associated with any male gave 40 - KK females in F,, and another, also parthenogenetically, gave 14 females and 1 male, all KK’s. The following F, KK female was mated with an RY male and gave KR 75:1299, KY 95:1399, and 32 9? of the pattern of the KK parent. Two of these F, KK females were further tested parthenogenetically and pro- duced, in F,, a total of 50 offspring, all like themselves. Two of the KY 99 were bred parthenogenetically and gave, both combined, 1 AA:12KK:10YY: 2KYKY, all females, thus exhibiting the crossovers AA and KYKY, as well as the expected segregates KK and YY. 136 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIH The five next F, KK females placed with Paratettix males produced 1, 2, 8, 9 and 22 offspring, respectively, all KK females. The sixth female of this group, ex- posed to no male whatsoever, behaved precisely as did those individuals which had been with Paratettix males. Another of these F, KK females was placed with an MT male with 4 KK female offspring resulting in F, none evidencing any M or T part in the par- entage. Three of these were bred further and gave off- spring in F, as follows: one, without any male, 2 KK females; the second, mated to an MM male, 3 KM females showing the male parentage unmistakably; and the third, mated to a YY male, 1 KY female showing male par- entage clearly, and 3 KK females indicating none of the male characteristics and plainly parthenogenetic. On account of the extremely small numbers involved, I sug- gest there is no special significance to the absence of males in the two last described matings. An eighth F, KK female, of this group, was mated to an OY male and gave KO 1155:2199 and KY 1365: 26 99. The following two matings show RR males mated to KM females and giving KR 27 64:26 992, MR 214: 35 99 and the crossovers KMR 16:19. Three of the KM females were placed with Paratettix males, but the off- spring exhibited no more evidence of male parentage than those produced by the two KM sisters without ex- posure to any male. The combined result from the five KM sisters was AA 1:KK 26:MM 38: KMKM 1, all fe- males, except that the sex of the AA individual was not determined. As in the ease of the KY females already noted, this furnishes a very interesting exhibition of segregation as well as crossing-over in parthenogenetic individuals. Subsequently from KK females, individually and in groups, but not exposed to males of any kind at any time, there have been given 2,726 female and 4 male offspring, all KKs, some of them having arrived at the fifth par- thenogenetic generation. Including the KK females rep- No. 625] PARTHENOGENESIS AND CROSSING-OVER tor resented in the chart, and others exposed to males with- out effect, there have been produced parthenogenetically from KK females, a total of 3,289 females and 5 males, all of the KK pattern. Other females than KK (some of the data presented below) have produced 1,181 fe- males and 2 males, making a total of 4,470 females and 7 males of various patterns produced parthenogenetically (August 1, 1918). Individuals of all the patterns, except AA which has not been adequately tested, have given off- spring by parthenogenesis. It is not known if any one breeds in this way more readily than any other, the KK’s having been used up to the present time more than the rest. Furthermore, it has not been determined whether, or not the capacity for parthenogenesis is in any sense an inheritable character. There are, however, great dif- ferences among individuals in this respect, even from the same F,, or F,, parthenogenetic batch. Note: ġġ on the left. 99 on the right. mo 2g RR? 2 eg 29 ag 992 uren Q MM RR YY Yuyz MRMR 0-1 0-1 0-2 0-18 0-1 0-1 0-16 0-4 0-7 0-16 0-7 0-61 0-5 0-19 Total 0-17 0-12 0-7 0-22 Total 0-44 0-15 0-24 0-8 0-37 0-85 Total 0-41 0-2 Total 0-96 Total 2-168 (6) (7) ER FẸ KY? F AA KK- MM KMKM AA KK Y¥ KYKY 18 oi 08 pA 0-0 06 0-2 00 0-0-0 05 O05 00 6) 68 68 0-0 0-0-0 0-13 0-12 0-0 0-0 -01i 00 oO Total 0-0-0 0-19 0-20 OI oo 04 08 00 From Chart 0-0-1 0-26 0-88 0-1 Total 0-0 0-14 0-14 0-0 Total 0-0-1 0-45 0-58 0-2 From Chart 0-1 0-12 0-10 0-2 Total 0-1 0-26 0-24 0-2 138 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST @ [VoL. LIH (8) (9) KYZ 29 ma Q KK Yuvu KYKY zz Ax MM RR MRMR 0-3 0-11 0—0 0-1 0-2 0-11 0—4 0-2 Total 0-21 0-25 0-2 0-8 A few other results from breeding Apotettix females parthenogenetically are given in the above tables, which also include the segregation and crossing-over in the KY and KM females shown in the chart. Further crossing-over is indicated in the partheno- genetic KYZ females (8) and the MR female (9). The latter indicates more than 21 per cent. of crossing-over, but the total crossing-over shown in 296 offspring from MR females, produced both bisexually and partheno- genetically, amounts to only 20, or less than 7 per cent. few other simple hybrid females, some partheno- genetically and others bisexually, have produced cross- overs as follows: | From GM females 279 individuals with 11 crossovers, about 4 per cent. From KM females 517 individuals with 5 crossovers, about 1 per cent. Y females 205 individuals with 12 crossovers, about 6 per cent. From RY females 33 individuals with 3 crossovers, about 10 per cent. From TY females 70 individuals with 8 crossovers, about 12 per cent. From RT females 125 individuals with no crossovers. There is every indication that as the numbers avail- able become larger these percentage figures will be dif- ferent; therefore it seems inadvisable to project at this time even a tentative diagram illustrating crossing-over percentages. CROSSING-OVER IN THE MALE Crossing-over in the females, in parthenogenetic as well as bisexual reproduction, is shown in the chart, and tables (1-9), and there are numerous other cases to be presented later in both bisexual and parthenogenetic breeding. While it appears that the crossovers in the females greatly exceed those in the males, the data are No. 625] P4RTHENOGENESIS AND CROSSING-OVER 139 as yet insufficient to justify a final judgment. A few of the considerable number of cases of crossing-over in males are herewith given: 0) ap (12) AYE BAK Ore ee Ma x Ox AK AYZ KYZ AZ KO KTY KT + GM GR KM KR KMR 3-2 3-2. 2-3 6-1 0-1 9-12 11-17 2-1 14 35 1-5 12 0-1 oer ig) net: oes ae es aes eae eae oe Woes Bicone iB a3) a4) KM x RR KYM X RT ÉMRARRR MR MT KYR KYT KMT 6-7-5 3- 44 0-1 0-2 8-15 14-9 16-20 144 0-1 SMe See 8, a Se 23 opps | Wee | gees The KM x RR mating (13) is of interest and a sample of a frequent occurrence. The KMR individual, of un- mistakable pattern, could not be accounted for otherwise than by assuming crossing-over in the male, but since AR and RR were so much alike and were not bred further, we can not know whether both of these were AR, the A gamete coming from crossing-over in the male, or whether they were RR produced parthenogenetically by the RR parent, or one was produced by the former and the other by the latter method. Discussion’ Are the female gametes in this group of grouse locusts all of the same kind with respect to the necessity of fer- tilization, or do some of them require the spermatozoon, in order to develop, and others not? The latter situation is suggested by the fact that mated individuals frequently reproduce bisexually and parthenogenetically at the Same time. Also, often, when an individual which has been reproducing parthenogenetically is mated she thenceforth gives offspring, some exhibiting, and others not, male parentage. In an unmated female perhaps the eggs that require fertilization disintegrate either before or after oviposition. (The eggs are oviposited in the 140 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Vou. LIII ground. This matter can be, and is being, investigated.) On the other hand, it as often happens that when an in- dividual reproducing parthenogenetically is mated she thenceforth gives offspring all showing male parentage. Also, though less readily, a mated female reproducing exclusively bisexually, when placed to herself, will, after a few weeks, give offspring parthenogenetically. Al- though the end result data which might give light on this point have not as yet been adequately developed, I ven- ture the suggestion that with respect to the need of fer- tilization the mature eggs are approximately the same, and that it is the time of the entrance of the sperma- tozoon which determines the matter. If a spermatozoon enters the egg at the proper stage of its maturation the pronuclei unite; if no spermatozoon enters, or one enters too late, the egg either proceeds parthenogenetically, or fails to develop altogether. The diploid number of chromosomes in the A potettix female appears to be fourteen. This number has been clearly demonstrated in the late metaphase plate of an oogonial division in an individual derived bisexually. In a preparation of somatic cells of a female produced par- thenogenetically, nine apparently whole chromosomes and some fragments were observed in one (Mr. A. H. Hersh, unpublished.) The females of some other mem- bers of the Tettigide have fourteen chromosomes (Rob- ertson, 716). In some forms of the Tettigide the males have been shown to have thirteen chromosomes as the complete number (Harman, 715, and Robertson, 716). The ‘‘maneuvers of the chromosomes’’ theory of Mor- gan may very well account for the observed end results presented in this paper, though the possibility of some other explanation is not by any means excluded. Even if the results herein entitled ‘‘crossing-over’’ should at some time be found actually not to be connected with the maneuvers of the chromosomes, the term might still be retained as an adequate expression of whatever does occur. The discussion of the mechanism, physiological No. 625] PARTHENOGENESIS AND CROSSING-OVER 141 processes, or both, involved in the parthenogenesis must await further investigation. In nature, individuals of the pattern AA, of a mottled gray ground color, in striking contrast with, and much less conspicuous than, the rest, exceed in numbers all the others combined. This situation has been ac- counted for in the past by the assumption that the bril- liant patterns rendered the individuals possessing them so conspicuous that they more readily fell prey to en- emies, while the inconspicuous and protected AA indi- viduals were largely unmolested. Now, since it has been demonstrated that crossing-over among the forms of con- spicuous patterns produces the A gametes as well, the cause of the preponderance of AA patterns in nature may, in part at least, call for an entirely different ex- planation. AA may be the primitive form and the others have originated from it, by mutation or in some other way. The form AA seems to correspond to the so-called ‘normal’? or ‘‘ wild type,’’ though all the others have also been found exclusively in nature, none (in Apotettix) having so far originated in the laboratory. CoNCLUSIONS 1. Through complete isolation of females from males of any kind, in some cases for as many as three genera- tions, and, in addition, by genetic behavior, it has been demonstrated that these forms of Apotettix are gyno- genetically, except rarely parthenogenetic (tychoparthe- nogenetic). 2. Segregation is demonstrated as occurring in hetero- zygous individuals reproducing by parthenogenesis, as well, and apparently to the same extent, as in those females reproducing bisexually. 3. Crossing-over is demonstrated as occurring in het- erozygous individuals reproducing by parthenogenesis, as well, and apparently to the same extent, as in those females reproducing bisexually. 4. Crossing-over occurs in the male, as well, but ap- parently not to the same extent, as in the female. 142 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LIII LITERATURE CITED re A. W. 1917. Studies of Inheritance pe Evolution in Orthop- i Journal si aea Vol. VII, No. RS J. 1902. e Tettigidae of N. A. iila -m eni F: 183. Spermatogenesis in Paratettix. Biol. Bulletin, Vol. XXIX, No. eo T. H and TIR 1915. Mechanism of Mendelian Heredity. New Yor parae Robert K. 1914. Studies of Inheritance and Evolution in Orth- Journal of Genetics, Vol. 1917. Stu y f: LEL No:8, : Seas Robert K, Inheritance and eget in Orth- optera IT and ont Journal of Nie VOL VIL NO: L Robertson, W. R. 1916. Chromosome ‘Studies I. Jour. Morphology, Vol. XXVII, = 2. THE EVOLUTION OF ARTHROPODS AND THEIR RELATIVES WITH ESPECIAL REFERENCE TO INSECTS! G. C. CRAMPTON, Px.D. MASSACHUSETTS AGRICULTURAL COLLEGE THe two lines of descent which have culminated in the production of some of the most active and ‘‘dynamic”’ of living creatures, and those in which the psychic facul- ties have attained their highest degree of perfection, are represented by the vertebrate group Mammalia, at whose apex is man, and the invertebrate group Arthropoda, at whose apex are the Insecta. Since these are the great rival groups, contending for the possession of the earth, the tracing of the paths by which they have arrived at their present dominating positions affords one of the most fascinating chapters of the study of organic evolu- tion. Concerning the ancestry of man, there is some degree of agreement of opinion in modern works upon the subject; but the recent investigations of Handlirsch, | 1904-1908, are not in accord with those of his predeces- sors in the study of insect phylogeny, and since his views have received a surprisingly widespread acceptance, his work has served to revive the discussion of the ultimate derivation of the insectan type of animals. There can be but little doubt that the insects and their arthropodan relatives are the descendants of ancestors related to the worm-like forms. These in turn were de- rived from lower worm-like ancestors resembling the members of the Rotifera-Platyhelminthes group. In the present state of our knowledge of the subject, it is hardly possible to state with any degree of certainty whether the ancestors of the worm-like forms were ultimately 1 Contribution from the ORE Laboratory of the Massachusetts Agricultural College, Amherst, Mass 143 144 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. LIII derived from the Celenterata-Porifera group—i. e., from a celenterate (cnidarian ?) type of animal through Ctenophora-like (?) forms; or more directly from a colonial protozoan type through forms comparable to the ‘‘Mesozoa’’ and their relatives, such as Dicyema, ete.; although there is a strong probability that the lower worm-like forms arose from ancestors occupying a posi- tion intermediate between these types of animals. From the standpoint of evolution, the Rotifera and Platyhel- minthes (also such worms as Dinophilus, ete.) are among the most important of the lower worm-like forms, since they have departed as little as any from the condition characteristic of the ancestors of the ‘‘Annelida’’ and many other invertebrates; and even the line of descent of the vertebrates themselves may ultimately lead back to forms not unlike the members of this group. ‘A higher stage of development is represented by the ‘‘ Annelida”? (including the Sternaspidide, Gephyrea, etc.), which are a group of the greatest phylogenetic importance due to the fact that their line of development is approached by, or is paralleled by, those of many other invertebrate groups, and to the fact that they have retained a great number of characteristics apparently typical of the an- cestors of the Arthropoda. Their forebears probably occupied a position intermediate between the Rotifera and the Platyhelminthes, and indeed, some investigators have even placed the ‘‘archiannelid’’ Dinophilus among the planarian Platyhelminthes, although its closest affini- ties seem to be with the annelidan worms Protodrilus and Polygordius. From their annelid-like forebears, there have branched off two important lines of descent, which have ap- proached very close to the arthropodan type, and which have even been classed among the Arthropoda by some investigators. One of these lines of development is rep- resented by the Onychophora, which are suggestively arthropod-like in many particulars, although they have retained many annelidan characters; while the other line No. 625] THE EVOLUTION OF ARTHROPODS 145 is represented by the Myzostomida, which are regarded by some authorities as occupying a position intermediate between the chetopod Annelida and the Tardigrada. The position of the Tardigrada is still a subject of con- siderable discussion, and the decision of the matter de- pends largely upon the settling of the question whether the apparent simplicity of their organization is due to the retention of a primitively simple condition, or to a secondarily acquired simplicity brought about by reduc- tion or degeneracy, ete. If the simplicity of the Tardi- grada is a primitive one, there is much to be said in favor of placing them next to the Myzostomida in the Myzo- stomida-Onychophora group; but whether the Lingua- tulida should also be included in this group seems very doubtful. From the resemblance of the adults to Eriophyid mites, and of the immature forms to such short-bodied mites as Phytoptus, ete., many recent authorities would place the Linguatulida near the Acarina—a highly modified group far removed from the base of the arthropodan stem. If this view is correct, the apparent simplicity of the Lin- guatulida is to be regarded as the result of a reduction or degeneration rather than the retention of a primitive condition, in forms so far removed. from the base of the arthropodan stem; and if the simplicity of the Tardigrada is primitive, while that of the Linguatulida has been sec- ondarily acquired through reduction or degeneracy, the apparent resemblance between the Tardigrada and Linguatulida must be regarded as the result of conver- gence, or parallelism, rather than of consanguinity. ` Under these conditions the Linguatulida could not be grouped with the Tardigrada, if the latter are placed next to the Myzostomida in the Myzostomida-Onycho- phora group; but if the apparent simplicity of the Tardi- grada is also due to reduction or degeneracy, they too might be placed with the Linguatulida near the mites— although this does not appear to be very probable from our present knowledge of the subject. It is quite ap- 146 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Von. LII parent, however, that the matter needs considerable further investigation before this question can be definitely determined. The affinities of the molluscan group are somewhat obscure, but the study of their immature stages would indicate that the Mollusea are rather distantly related to the Chetopoda, Gephyrea, ‘‘Polyzoa,’’ and other annelid-like forms. If this be true, their line of develop- ment apparently likewise extends back to forebears simi- lar to the members of the Rotifera-Platyhelminthes group (which are very like the ancestors of the ‘‘ Anne- lida’’ also), and the ancestors of the Echinodermata and Hemichordata may possibly be traced back to a similar stock (or to forms closely related to them) as well. The Hemichordata are regarded by many authorities as a group which has departed but little structurally from the ancestral condition of the forms leading up to the vertebrate type of animal. From a study of their larve, some investigators have concluded that the Hemi- chordata are related to the Echinodermata; but this brings us no nearer to the solution of the problem, since the Echinodermata occupy an isolated position, and their ultimate affinities are very obscure. Although the Echi- noderm line of development may lead back more directly to the Coelenterata, there is a possibility of their fore- bears being quite closely related to the members of the Rotifera-Platyhelminthes group which have departed but little from the condition characteristic of the ances- tors of the ‘‘ Annelida ’’ and other invertebrate groups; -and even if the Echinodermata are to be regarded as the descendants of Celenterata-like forebears, it must be re- membered that the ancestors of the Rotifera-Platyhel- minthes group were themselves very closely related to the Coelenterata, and would probably have been classed as such, rather than as colonial Protozoa. It is quite possible to regard the line of development of the Echinodermata as related to the other two lines of development in question, and as branching off near, or No. 625] . THE EVOLUTION OF ARTHROPODS 147 even further down in the developmental scale, than the points of origin of the lines of descent of the Hemichor- data and the worm-like forms. It is also possible to suppose that in the hemichordatan line there have been carried over certain developmental tendencies from their common ancestry, such as the preservation of a ‘‘tor- naria’’ larva characteristic of the Echinodermata, while in other respects the Hemichordatan line of development has paralleled that of the worm-like forms more closely, having taken over more of the tendencies which were to find opportunities for fuller expression in the worm-like forms from their ultimately common inheritance. Under these conditions the Hemichordata are related to both the Echinodermata and the worm-like forms, but their line of development has accompanied that of the worm- like forms much more closely and for a longer distance before each branched off along its own path of speciali- zation. If the lines of development of the Hemichordata and of the worm-like forms have an ultimately common ancestry, and if both lines of descent have ‘‘travelled along the same developmental road’’ for a considerable distance before each branched off along its own path of specialization, it is not surprising that we find many structural resemblances in the two lines of descent, and the resemblance of such Hemichordata as Cephalodiscus, Rhabdopleura, Phoronis, etc., to certain ‘‘ Polyzoa,?’ may be as much the result of consanguinity as of ‘‘convergent development.’’ This view enables us to harmonize the apparently discordant theories concerning the ultimate origin of the Vertebrata—all of which may contain a por- tion of the truth, as is frequently the case in the different hypotheses put forward to explain certain observed phe- nomena. Thus, according to this conception, we may derive the Vertebrata from forms similar to the Hemi- chordata, and still account for the annelid-like (and arachnid-like) features which appear in certain of the lower representatives of the vertebrate group, since tendencies present in the ancestors which ultimately 148 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII gave rise to both the Annelida and the Hemichordata are quite likely to appear in both Annelida and Hemi- chordata (or in forms descended from them, such as the Arachnida and the Vertebrata).? One of the chief difficulties in the way of reaching a proper understanding of the mutual interrelationships of the different lines of development is the attempt to arrange these lines in the form of a dichotomously branching tree drawn in one plane—which is almost as impossible as the attempt to arrange all animals in a single linear developmental series; for it’ must be borne in mind that these different lines of development frequently approach one another from different directions, so that it would be necessary to represent their relationships by a figure drawn in three dimensions, rather than in a single plane. If this is done, it becomes easier to under- stand that the line of development of the ‘‘Annelida,’’ . for example, is paralleled (on different sides) by those of several other groups, and that all of these lines of descent may lead back to a common ancestry, or that their points of origin may be near the point at which the line of descent of the ‘‘ Annelida’’ arose. In discussing the ultimate relationships of the Verte- brata, Echinodermata, Mollusea, ete., the lack of inter- mediate forms annectent between the different develop- mental series, or connecting them with the supposedly ancestral forms, has made the subject of their affinities extremely speculative; and it is not until we come to the consideration of arthropod phylogeny that the evidence is at all satisfactory, and even here important gaps in the developmental chain leave much to be desired. As was previously stated, the members of the Myzosto- mida-Onychophora group have developed many charac- ters strongiy suggestive of arthropod affinities; but they 2 This statement should not be interpreted as implying that recent ver- tebrates are descended from living Hemichordata, etc., but it is merely intended to indicate that the Hemichordata have departed but little from the probable ancestral condition of the Vertebrata, and the same holds true for similar statements throughout this paper. No. 625] THE EVOLUTION OF ARTHROPODS 149 have become too greatly modified along their own lines of specialization in regard to those particular structures most frequently used in comparative morphology to be of much value for a phylogenetic study of the develop- ment of the different parts of the body in the lower arthropods. Among the ‘‘Annelida,’’ on the other hand, we find some very promising material for such a study, especially among the chetopodan annelids, such as the Syllide (e. g., Dujardinia rotifera, ete.), which have seg- mented appendages, while others of the group have de- veloped structures no less interesting from the stand- point of phylogeny, indicating that they have departed but little from the ancestral condition of the arthropods. The segmentation of the body of these annelids,* the nature and relative positions of the heart and the di- gestive, nervous and other systems, very readily lend themselves to such an interpretation, and it is not a diff- cult matter to derive the head region of a primitive arthropod from that of the annelid type (Bernard, 1892), or to derive the appendages of such an arthropod from those of the annelidan type, as has been recently dis- cussed by Borradale, 1917. In connection with the discussion of the derivation of the appendages of the lower arthropods from structures comparable to the parapodia of the annelids, it may be remarked that the attempt of Lankester, 1872, to derive the Arthropoda more directly from the Rotifera, such as the remarkable Pedalion mira (whose appendages and the ‘‘arms’’ of the male Asplanchna he compares to the movable spines of Triarthra and Polyarthra), has not been productive of as important results as those ob- tained from the comparison of the annelidan structures with those of the arthropods. This, however, is merely to be expected, since the annelids have developed far 3 Just as some annelids are many-segmented, while the bodies of others are composed of fewer segments, it is quite reasonable to suppose that the ancestors of .the arthropods exhibited a considerable range in the number of segments composing their bodies—and even among the most primitive arthropods there is a wide range in the number of segments composing their bodies. 150 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vor. LIII more features in common with the lower arthropods than have such primitive forms as the Rotifera. On the other hand, the Annelida themselves (and hence ultimately the Arthropoda also) are the descendants of Rotifera-like (and Platyhelminthes-like) forebears, and it is quite possible that certain rotifers might develop features which later find opportunity for fuller expression in the forms descended from them (e. g., the striated muscles of Pedalion); but, since the general organization of a rotifer’s body is not so similar to that of the lower arthropods as is the case in the annelids in question, for the present at least it seems preferable to regard the slight resemblances between the appendages of the Rotif- . era and the lower Arthropoda mentioned above as the result of ‘‘convergence’’ (parallelism) in development rather than to consider it as a precocious development of structures later to be developed in the arthropod de- scendants of ancestors ultimately arising from rotiferan forebears. Iwould not utterly deny the latter possibility, however, since it may be quite possible that arthropods are to be derived more directly from Rotifera-like fore- bears (e. g., Hexarthra polyptera, ete.) through forms related to the Tardigrada and Nauplius-like ancestors; but the great mass of evidence from comparative anat- omy, embryology, etc., points to an ‘‘annelid ancestry” for the Arthropoda, and until other hitherto undiscov- ered forms have been found to indicate some other deri- vation for the group, we are safe in assuming that the ‘*Annelida’’ represent as nearly as any known forms the ancestral condition of the Arthropoda. In taking up the consideration of the evolution of the Arthropoda themselves, the question naturally arises as to what arthropods have departed the least from the probable ancestral condition of the group as a whole. Some investigators would claim that since the Nauplius larva is of such widespread occurrence among the lower arthropods, that it represents an ancestral type; but it must be borne in mind that a free-swimming larva is No. 625] THE EVOLUTION OF ARTHROPODS 151 usually very greatly modified in adaptation to its own mode of life and environmental conditions, and fre- quently represents an interpolated stage having no great -phylogenetic significance (in comparison with the devel- opmental stages of the embryo). Furthermore, it is extremely probable that the ancestral arthropods were not of one single type at all, but doubtless differed very greatly among themselves in size, the number of seg- ments composing their bodies, ete., just as is the case among certain annelidan groups, or as is the case among the assemblage of lower arthropods comprising the most primitive members of the group next to be discussed. The assemblage of lower arthropods comprising the Copepoda, Branchiopoda and their immediate relatives may be referred toas the Copepoda-Branchiopoda group. Its members include some of the most primitive of the arthropods, and it may be regarded as representing as nearly as any the forms giving rise to the different arthropodan lines of development. The Ostracoda rep- resent a line of development which branched off at an early date, and should also be included in the group; but they are not structurally so important as the Branchio- poda, etc., for a phylogenetic study of the lines of descent to which the ancestral arthropoda gave rise. The Cir- repedia likewise represent a group which branched off from this stem at an early date, but they are too degen- erate, and have followed their own line of specialization too far to be included among the primitive representa- tives of the Copepoda-Branchiopoda group. The Trilo- bita are very closely related to the Apodie and Branchio- poda in general, for such trilobites as Nathorstia transi- tans are somewhat annectent between the trilobites and the branchiopod Opabina regalis described by Walcott, 1912, and such trilobites as Marella splendens are very like certain Apodide, ete.; but the closest affinities of the Trilobita appear to be with the group next to be consid- ered, and although the trilobites have preserved many very primitive features which might entitle them to a 152 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vot LIH position in the ancestral ‘‘Copepoda-Branchiopoda’’ group, it is preferable to consider them as members of the Trilobita-Merostomata group, with which they have much more in common. The Trilobita-Merostomata group is composed of the Trilobita, Eurypterida, and Xiphosura, with their imme- diate relatives, and includes the forms which have de- parted the least from the ancestral condition of the arachnoids in general. The Trilobita are extremely closely related to the Merostomata, some of which (such for example as the fossil merostome Bunodes lunula, which has been admirably restored by Patten, 1912, or the cambrian merostomes, Sidneyia inexpectans, Emeral- della brocki, ete., described by Walcott, 1911-1912) bear well-developed antennæ very similar to those of the trilo- bites; so that the division of the Arthropoda into ‘‘Te- leiocerata’’ and ‘‘Chelicerata’’ by Heymons, 1901, or into ‘‘Antennata’’ and ‘‘Chelicerota’’ by Boerner, 1909, can not be strictly applied when we take these forms into consideration. Walcott, 1912, considers that such mero- stomes as Molaria spinifera are connected with the trilo- bites through such intermediate forms as Nathorstia transitans—a trilobite also related to the Branchiopoda. Walcott also considers that the merostome Sidneyia rep- resents a transition form between the trilobites and the eurypterids, and that the merostomes Beltina and Sid- neyia are related to the ancestors of living Xiphosura; so that according to his views the trilobites are descended from branchiopods, while the eurypterids are descended from trilobites through such merostomes as his ‘‘Agla- spina” and ‘‘Limulava,’’ from which living Xiphosura are descended. In discussing the lower arachnoid forms, it is neces- sary to take into consideration the Pantopoda, which have apparently retained certain features strongly sug- gestive of crustacean affinities, while certain other fea- tures suggest that they are related to the arachnoid forms. Boerner, 1902, however, thinks that the Panto- No. 625] THE EVOLUTION OF ARTHROPODS 153 poda are structurally quite far removed from the arach- nids examined by him, and since he has made a very extensive study of the different arachnoid forms, his opinion should have considerable weight. Since their line of development does not approach very closely to those of the other forms here discussed, the study of the Pantopoda is not of as great phylogenetic importance as that of those forms which occupy a position annectent between the other groups, or whose lines of descent ap- proach those of the other groups. For the purpose of the present paper, it is therefore sufficient to say that the Pantopoda represent a highly aberrant group whose line of descent branched off at an early date, somewhere near the Trilobita-Merostomata group, and that they have followed a widely divergent path of specialization. The scorpions are descended from forms very like the eurypterid members of the Trilobita-Merostomata group, and such eurypterids as Glyptoscorpius occupy a posi- tion annectent between the two groups. On the other hand, the scorpions, together with the Pedipalpi, are in many respects very like the ancestors of the higher arachnids, so that they form an ancestral group, the Scorpionida-Pedipalpi, intermediate between the Trilo- bita-Merostomata group and the higher arachnids. In the Scorpionida-Pedipalpi. group should be included the closely allied pseudoscorpions and probably the Koene- nia-like forms and the Solifuge also. Of the higher groups of arachnids, the spiders (Ara- nee) are apparently quite closely related to the ambly- pigid (tarantulid) branch of the Pedipalpi, while the Phalangidea (Opiliones) and Acarina are more closely related to the pseudoscorpions and Solifugæ, although it has been suggested that the Cryptostemmatide occupy a position intermediate between the Pedipalpi (to which they are somewhat more closely allied) and the Phalangi- dea. The Linguatulida have been placed near the mites by many recent authorities on account of the supposed resemblance of their larve to such short-bodied mites 154 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII as Phytoptus, ete., and the apparent resemblance of the adults to eriophyid mites, so that provisionally, at least, the Linguatulida may be regarded as strongly aberrant mites, while the Tardigrada are probably not related to them, but to the Myzostomida, as has been previously discussed. Since the arachnoid path of evolution has led off in a direction widely divergent from the path followed in the development of the Insecta, it is very difficult to under- stand how Thorell came to the conclusion that such highly developed arachnids (i. e., those occupying a posi- tion far along the divergent line of development) as the Solifuge are intimately related to insects. Further- more, since the trilobite trend of development leads off toward the merostomes and the divergent evolutionary path of the arachnoid forms, it is necessary to search further back than the trilobites for a group standing more nearly in the direct line of development eventually resulting in the evolution of the insectan type, and for this purpose the study of the branchiopod representa- tives of the Copepoda-Branchiopoda group is much more valuable. The members of the Copepoda-Branchiopoda group which seem to be the nearest to the stem forms at the base of the line of descent which ultimately leads up to the insect type of development are the Notostraca (Apo- did) and anostracan Branchiopoda, which are likewise very closely related to the trilobites, so that certain an- cestral features are to be found in the trilobites also, having been inherited from their common forebears; but, as was stated above, the trend of trilobite development is toward the production of the eurypterid and arachnoid type of development, and therefore leads away from the line of development which eventually results in the pro- duction of the insect type. Walcott, 1912, agrees with Bernard, 1892, in regarding the Apodide as among the lowest representatives of the Arthropoda (although cer- tain copepods are also extremely primitive) and suggests No. 625] THE EVOLUTION OF ARTHROPODS 155 that the fossil annelids, Canadia spinosa (in which the ` head is bent down ‘‘so that the mouth faces posteriorly”? in the position assumed by Bernard, 1892, to be that of the annelids which gradually took on the character of head region leading up to the arthropod type), and the Crustacea ‘‘were derived from the same general type of animal.” The Copepoda represent a line of develop- ment which branched off near that of the Branchiopoda, at the base of the arthropod stem; and the Argulide (which are grouped with the Copepoda by Calman, 1909) are regarded by some authorities as annectent between the Copepoda and the Branchiopoda. The Ostracoda are related to both the conchostracan and cladoceran Branchiopoda (following Calman’s classification) and the ancestors of the ostracods doubtless arose from forms intermediate between the Cladocera and Conchostraca. The Cirrepedia are apparently descended from ancestors related to both the Ostracoda and Copepoda, and their line of development branched off at an early date to fol- low their own strongly aberrant part of development. ‘Such anostracan branchiopods as the fossil Opabina regalis, whose structure according to Walcott, 1912, ‘‘is very suggestive of an annelidan ancestor,’ and such notostracan branchiopods as the fossil Burgessia bella (which has sessile eyes and hepatic glands in a‘ carapace resembling that of Lepidurus) serve to indicate what the first arthropods were probably like, and they occupy a - position near the base of the stem-forms whose lines of development were eventually to produce the insectan type of arthropod. The fossil notostracan branchiopod Waptia occupies a position annectent between the above- mentioned branchiopods and the malacostracan group next to be considered. The leptostracan (phyllocarid) group occupies a po- sition intermediate between the rest of the Malacostraca and the branchiopods described above. They have also carried over from their common branchiopod ancestry certain features likewise inherited by the trilobites; but. 156 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. LII as was previously stated, the trilobites do not stand in the direct line of descent of the Leptostraca, and those characters which they possess in common were inherited from their common branchiopod ancestry, and can not be interpreted as indicating that the trilobites represent the ancestral forms giving rise to the lepostracan type. The fossil leptostracan Hymenocaris is evidently related to the fossil branchiopod Waptia (which occupies a posi- tion intermediate between the branchiopods and Lepto- traca), but Hymenocaris is clearly a leptostracan, and resembles such living forms as Nebalia, while the fossil leptostracans Carnavonia and Tuzoia resemble such liv- ing Leptostraca as Nebaliopsis typica in the character of the carapace, etc. The closer affinities of the fossil Ceratiocaride, ete., have not been determined, due to the imperfect preservation of the limbs, ete., but they clearly belong to the leptostracan group. There is much to be said in favor of including the Leptostraca in the next group of the Malacostraca to be considered, but from the standpoint of a phylogenetic study it is preferable to consider the Leptostraca (together with other primitive forms not yet described) as nearer the ancestral forms from which the other Malacostraca were derived. A further stage of development is represented by the Anomostraca-Cumacea group which includes the Synca- rida and a portion of the Peracarida of Calman’s classi- fication, together with their immediate relatives. The Anomostraca (Anaspidacea and Bathynellacea of Chap- pius, 1915), Mysidacea and Cumacea are very closely interrelated, and all of them exhibit affinities with the Leptostraca, so that the members of the leptostracan group might well be included here also; but they have been treated as a separate group, to emphasize the fact that they occupy a position annectent between the Branchiopoda and the Malacostraca (with which their strongest affinities lie). Although the members of Ano- mostraca-Cumacea group are extremely closely related to the Tanaidacea, the closest affinities of the Tanaidacea No. 625] THE EVOLUTION OF ARTHROPODS 157 are with the Isopoda (and Amphipoda), so that it is pref- erable to consider them with the latter group. The Ano- mostraca-Cumacea group is of the greatest phylogenetic importance, since its members have departed as little as any known forms from the probable ancestral condition of the higher Crustacea, Insecta and ‘‘Myriopoda’’ (sensu lato). The Mysidacea have retained some primitive charac- ters indicating their connection with the Leptostraca- - like forms which preceded them, and they are quite like the ancestors of the eucaridan (euphausiacean and deca- podan) members of the higher crustacean groups. They are also probably related more remotely to the ancestors of the aberrant hoplocaridan (stomatopodan) line of de- velopment, and through such forms as the Cumacea they are connected with the ancestors of the Tanaidacea (and therefore of the Isopoda also). They are not so impor- tant for a phylogenetic study of the insects, etc., how- ever, as the Anomostraca and Cumacea (with the Tanai- dacea) are. The Cumacea occupy a position interme- diate between the Mysidacea and the Tanaidacea, being somewhat more closely allied to the latter. They are also related to the Anomostraca (Syncarida), as is true of the Mysidacea, the interrelations of the different members of the group being rather complicated. From the standpoint of the study of the phylogeny of the insects and their relatives the Anomostraca and Cu- macea (together with the Tanaidacea) are by far the most important forms, since the ancestors of the insects and their relatives were doubtless descended from forms closely related to the Anomostraca, Cumacea, and Tanai- dacea. Of these three, the Anomostraca are apparently the most ancient (fossil remains of the others have not yet been found), and have doubtless departed as little as any from the ancestral forms which were eventually to give rise to the isopods, insects and ‘‘myriopods.”’ The fossil Pleuroearide (e. g., Acanthotelson, ete.) are nearer to the living genera Koonunga, Anaspides, Para- 158 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von LIH naspides, etc., while the fossil ‘‘Gampsonychide’’ (e. g., “Gampsonyx,” Paleocaris and Gasocaris) are nearer the living genus Bathynella. Such fossil forms as Prea- naspides found in the Carboniferous rocks is extremely like the living Anaspides which has apparently pre- served many ancestral characters, but little modified, to the present time. The Anomostraca are related to the Leptostraca, but no forms intermediate between them and the Leptostraca have yet been described, and it is possible that the line of descent of the Anomostraca leads . back to the branchiopods through Leptostraca-like forms not yet discovered. Superficially, at least, such slender branchiopods as Yohoia tenuis, ete., resemble certain members of the Anomostraca, and it is possible that the slenderer, more cylindrical Anomostraca, such as Bathy- nella, may have inherited the tendency toward the slen- der form of body from anostracan branchiopods of the Yohoia type. In Bathynella the eyes have become com- pletely lost, but in Koonunga sessile eyes are found and their presence suggests that sessile-eyed forms may have developed from the Koonunga type. In Anaspides the eyes are stalked. From ancestors occupying a position intermediate be- tween the Anomostraca and Cumacea (and also related to the Mysidacea) have arisen the lines of descent leading to the isopod Crustacea, Insecta, and ‘‘Myriopoda’”’ (s. 1.). The Tanaidacea (Chelifera) which occupy a posi- tion near the base of the isopod stem are very closely related to the Anomostraca, Cumacea and Mysidacea, and, together with the Isopoda and Amphipoda (which are descended from ancestors very similar to them), they might be included in the Anomostraca-Cumacea group; but if the Isopoda-Amphipoda group is considered sepa- rately, the Tanaidacea must be included in the latter group, since their closest affinities are with the Isopoda. The Amphipoda are quite closely related to the Isopoda, and their ancestors may also have arisen from forms intermediate between the Anomostraca and Cumacea No. 625] THE EVOLUTION OF ARTHROPODS 159 (and also related to the Mysidacea), so that the sessile- eyed character occurring in the group might be regarded as a retention of the tendency toward the formation of sessile eyes exhibited by such primitive forms as Koo- nunga, while the slender body form present in such Am- phipoda as the caprellids, Rhabdosoma, ete., may possi- bly be due to the retention of the tendency toward the slender form of body (such as that present in the more primitive Bathynella) in forms which are otherwise rather highly modified. The Isopoda-Amphipoda group origi- nated very close to the point of origin of the insect line of development, and the two lines have paralleled one another extremely closely. Since the’ members of the TIsopoda-Amphipoda group have not travelled so far along the path of specialization in following the same developmental road with the insects, they have retained many primitive features characteristic of the ancestors of the insects (and ‘‘myriopods’’), and such forms as Apseudes are particularly interesting for a phylogenetic study of insects and their immediate relatives. The Symphyla-Pauropoda group (composed of such forms as Scolopendrella, Scutigerella, Pauropus, Eury- pauropus, and their immediate relatives) contains the forms which appear to be very near the base of the ‘‘myriopod’’ stem, and which have retained a great num- ber of features characteristic of the ancestors of insects, so that a study of the structures of the Isopoda-Amphi- poda group and the Symphyla-Pauropoda group are of the greatest importance for a proper conception of the nature of the first insects to be evolved. The Symphyla- Pauropoda group probably also arose from forms occu- pying a position intermediate between the Anomostraca and Cumacea, and likewise closely related to the Tanai- dacea which originated from similar forebears. Such Anomostraca as Bathynella have not departed far from the ancestral condition of the Symphyla-Pauropoda group, and although they have developed many modifi- cations along their own line of specialization, they are as 160 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII near as any known forms to the ancestors of the Sym- phyla, ete. The Symphyla-Pauropoda group in turn has departed but slightly from the ancestral condition of the ‘‘Myriopoda’’ as a whole, although the ancestral ‘‘My- riopoda’’ comprised forms with bodies composed of more numerous segments as well as those made up of fewer segments. From ancestors similar to the mem- bers of the Symphyla-Pauropoda group one line of de- velopment has led to the chilopod type of myriopod, while the other has led to the diplopod type. From their ancestors related to the members of the Symphyla-Pau- ropoda group, the Chilopoda have carried over many characters also inherited by the ancestors of insects, so that a structural study of the Chilopoda is of consider- able value from the standpoint of insect phylogeny (as is true to a lesser degree of the Diplopoda also). As was stated above, the ancestors of the Insecta were related to the members of-both the Isopoda-Amphipoda group (including the Tanaidacea) and the Symphyla- Pauropoda group, so that the lines of descent of all three groups (insects, isopods and Symphyla) doubtless had a common origin in forms intermediate between the Cu- macea and Anomostraca (and also related to the Mysi- dacea), and all of the three groups have inherited from their common ancestry many characters also carried over in the lines of development of the other two of the three groups in question. The common ancestors of the three groups just mentioned (insects, isopods and Sym- phyla) were not of any one single type, but doubtless differed quite markedly among themselves in the number of segments composing their bodies, the slender or stouter and flatter character of the body and other fea- tures. Some of them were more like the Tanaidacea, while others were more like Bathynella and other mem- bers of the Anomostraca, ete., and this should be clearly borne in mind in attempting to determine what the an- cestors of the insects, ete., were like; for the greatest obstacle to arriving at the realization of the true nature No. 625] THE EVOLUTION OF ARTHROPODS 161 of the ancestors of insects and their relatives has been the attempt to derive them all from one type of creature —which is manifestly impossible, since even the lowest representatives of any group differ markedly among themselves, and their ancestors also must have differed markedly among themselves (although not to such a great extent as their progeny do). Although such Anomostraca as Bathynella have be- come specialized along their own lines of development, they have retained many features which suggest what some of the ancestors of the insects and Symphyla must have been like, and I think it very probable that the an- eestors of Scolopendrella and the Protura were quite similar in many respects to Bathynella, while other ap- terygotan insects, such as Machilis, have carried over more characters from the tanaidacean side of their com- mon ancestry. Therefore, if we accept the idea that some of the common ancestors of insects, isopods and Symphyla occupied a position intermediate between the lines of development of the Anomostraca and the Cu- macea-Tanaidacea, and differed a little less among them- selves than the Anomostraca do from the Cumacea- Tanaidacea, it becomes perfectly clear that some aptery- gotan insects could inherit from the tanaidacean side of their common ancestry characters which also appear in the isopods which are derived from Tanaidacea-like fore- bears; while on the other hand, other apterygotan insects could inherit from the Bathynella side of their common ancestry certain characters which also appear in the Symphyla or other forms descended from Bathynella- like forebears. The Protura (such as Acerentomon, Eosentomon, ete.) are the most primitive representatives of the Insecta, and have inherited from their common ancestry many features also preserved in the ‘‘Myriopoda’’; and the embryological development of the apterygotan group to which they belong has much in common with that of the **Myriopoda,”’ as has been pointed out by Philiptschenko, 162 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. LII 1912, Lignau, 1911, Chamberlain, 1917, Heymons, and others. The retention of the stumps of three pairs of legs on the abdominal region (in addition to the three pairs of thoracic legs) at first caused some zoologists to doubt that the Protura are really insects (since the idea that such forms with vestigial abdominal legs could not be true ‘‘hexapods’’ if they had more than six limbs seemed to stand in the way of their realizing the true insectan nature of the Protura), but the overwhelming evidence of their structural organization has convinced all recent entomologists that the Protura are true insects. As pointed out in a recent paper (Crampton, 1916) the Protura are quite closely related to such other Aptery- . gota as Tomocerus; and, with the Entomobryids and Sminthurids, they constitute the non-styli-bearing divi- sion of the Apterygota. Of the styli-bearing Apterygota, the next group to be considered, which may be referred to as the Campodeoid group, comprises the Rhabdura (e. g., Campodea), the Dicellura (e. g., Projapyx, Japyx, ete.) and their imme- diate relatives. Dicellura, such as Projapyx, Anajapy2, etc., have segmented cerci, and occupy a position inter- mediate between the Rhabdura, such as Campodea, and the other Dicellura, although their closest affinities are clearly with the Dicellura. The Campodeoid group, whose members have entognathous mouth parts and ves- tigial abdominal legs suggestive of the proturan struc- tures, occupy a position intermediate between the lower apterygotan Protura and the higher apterygotan forms, such as Nicoletia, Lepisma, ete., which also belong to the | styli-bearing apterygotan subdivision which includes the Campodeoid group as well (Crampton, 1916). The Cam- podeoid group, while inheriting certain features from the symphylan side of their common ancestry, have inherited in addition certain other features more typical of the crustacean side—which likewise reappear in the isopod- amphipod descendants of their common ancestors. The Lepismoid group, composed of the lepismids, No. 625] THE EVOLUTION OF ARTHROPODS 163 machilids, and their immediate relatives, is quite closely connected with the Campodeoid group in the styli-bear- ing subdivision of the Apterygota; but their mouth parts are ectognathous, and in their general organization they approach remarkably closely to the lower Pterygota; so that they may be said to occupy a position annectent between the lower Pterygota and the Campodeoid group. The members of the Lepismoid group seem to have in- herited more characters from the crustacean side of their common ancestry than from the symphylan side, while the members of the Proturan group seem to have inher- ited more characters from the symphylan side, and the members of the Campodeoid group appear to partake to some extent of characters occurring in both the crusta- cean (isopod) and symphylan sides of their common ancestry. It might be possible to explain the presence of both crustacean (isopod) and symphylan characters in the in- sectan stem by supposing that the crustacean, insectan and symphylan ‘‘currents’’ in the ‘‘onward flow of life,’’ although acquiring more and more of a distinct indi- viduality as their ‘‘waters’’ emerge from the common stream at their source, nevertheless have an intermin- gling or commingling of contiguous waters as they flow side by side, before ultimately diverging too greatly for such an intermingling. This idea, however, might in a sense be interpreted as meaning that the Symphyla-like insects were descended from Symphyla, and the Crus- tacea-like insects from Crustacea (i. e., isopod Crus- tacea), whereas insects as a whole were probably not ‘“polyphyletic,’’ but all insects were derived from a common ancestral source. The forms composing this common ancestral source, however, differed among them- selves very greatly, although the amount of divergence was probably not too great to prevent their being grouped in a single class—or possibly even in a single subclass or order. In this ancestral-insectan group, there were doubtless isopod-like insects which resembled the most 164 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LIII insect-like representatives of the ancestral isopods, while the Symphyla-like members of the ancestral-insectan group must have resembled the most insect-like repre- sentatives of the ancestral Symphyla. In other words, at the common level at which the lines of descent of the isopods, insects and Symphyla originated, some of the ancestral insects (which differed greatly among them- selves) occupying the ‘‘hereditary territory’’ contiguous to that of the ancestral Symphyla would inherit certain developmental tendencies in common with or similar to those also inherited by certain Symphyla; and similarly, some of the ancestral insects occupying the ‘hereditary territory” contiguous to that of the ancestral isopods would inherit certain developmental tendencies similar to those of certain isopods and the same principle would apply to successively larger, as well as to the smaller groups in any evolutionary study. According to this view, certain developmental or ‘‘inherent’’ tendencies exhibited by the isopods or myriopods might also appear in insects if the opportunity of manifesting themselves should arise, and this would merely imply that these tendencies were inherited from an ultimately common ancestry, rather than that some insects were descended from isopods while other insects were descended from Symphyla, ete. Some evolutionists might object to the use of such terms as ‘‘inherent tendencies’’ on the ground that they savor too strongly of ‘‘vitalism’’; but, so far as I can see, the expression ‘‘inherent tendencies’’ means much the same thing as a part of ‘‘heredity,’’ and one implies no more of a predilection toward vitalism than the other does. Although their closest affinities are with the Cam- podeoid group and the Apterygota in general, certain members of the Lepismoid group are structurally remark- ably similar in many respects to such primitive Ptery- gota as the stone-flies and may-flies, so that Handlirsch, 1906, who has completely disregarded the close interrela- tionships of the Apterygota, and their evident ancestral character (with reference to the winged insects) in his No. 625] THE EVOLUTION OF ARTHROPODS 165 attempt to derive the Pterygota more directly from trilo- bites, is forced to assume that the lepismids may represent degenerate Pterygota! Their whole sturctural organiza- tion clearly proclaims in no uncertain terms that the closest affinities of the lepismids are with the rest of the Apterygota, with which they are connected by inter- mediate forms, and a careful study of the comparative anatomy and embryology of the Apterygota, ‘‘ Myrio- poda” and Crustacea can result in no other conclusion than that the Apterygota have departed as little as any known forms from the condition characteristic of the an- cestors of the Pterygota. The lepismids are therefore no more to be considered as degenerate Pterygota, than apes are to be considered as degenerate men—unless one re- verses the whole scheme of evolution; and under such conditions there would be nothing to prevent any one from assuming that trilobites are degenerate lepismids, or any other equally improbable reversing of the evolutionary sequences ! In connection with the supposedly ‘‘degenerate’’ con- dition of the Apterygota, I would take issue with the im- plication carried in such statements as that by Tothill, 1916 (p. 376), who would claim that the Apterygota ‘‘are highly specialized animals as indicated by the frequent reduction of mouth parts, visual organs, tracheæ, ete. ; and by the development of peculiar structures such as the caudal spring and collophore.’’ In the first place, it is inadmissible to judge the ancestral character of any group by the condition of its most highly specialized members, as Tothill appears to do in the case of the Apterygota, since any arthropodan group, no matter how low it may be in the seale of development (e. g., Cope- poda, ete.) may include certain members which have be- come very highly specialized along their own lines of de- velopment without affecting the general position of the group as a whole; and in a phylogenetic study we must consider the most primitive representatives of the group, rather than the most highly specialized ones, if such a study is to yield any tangible results. If Tothill had 166 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [VoL LIH therefore considered such lowly organized Apterygota as - Eosentomon, Anajapyz, ete., instead of the highly special- ized Anurida, Sminthurus, ete., I am sure that his opinion of the “‘degenerate’’ condition of the Apterygota as com- pared with the Pterygota would have been quite the opposite of that expressed in his paper. Furthermore, there are practically no arthropods known which are primitive in all respects, and, as is the case throughout the whole realm of zoology, forms which have retained many features in an exceedingly primitive condition may be very highly specialized in other respects; so that one must take into consideration the composite primitive features of the group as a whole; and, just as the most primitive members of the Pterygota are studied in an attempt to determine their ancestry, so the most primi- tive members of the Apterygota must be considered in such a phylogenetic study. Even in the matter of the nature of their eyes, such forms as Machilis (which are related to Lepisma) can hardly be called ‘‘degenerate,’’ and in the face of the fact that in the trilobites themselves there occur at least three types of eyes—‘‘isolated eyes or ocelli, aggregate eyes of biconvex lenses, and compound eyes”? (Tothill, p. 321, quoted from Lindstrom, 1901), it is very improb- able that the type of eyes found in Lepisma are of a higher type than the compound eyes of the Pterygota. As far as their mouth parts are concerned, I find the lepismids much more primitive than the Pterygota (with the possible exception of nymphal ephemerids) and Boer- ner, 1908-1909, has called attention to crustacean struc- tures so similar to those found in the maxille, etc., of apterygotan insects, that there can be no doubt that the mouth parts of the Apterygota in general instead of being ‘‘degenerate’’ have retained many more primitive features than those of most lower Pterygota. As far as the number of abdominal segments is con- cerned, some Apterygota, instead of having fewer seg- ments, have even retained twelve, and in these forms, such as the Protura, there is also a postembryonie in- No. 625] THE EVOLUTION OF ARTHROPODS 167 crease in the number of segments (from nine to twelve in the abdomen) comparable to the increase of segments in the ‘‘Myriopoda,’’ so that Tothill’s statement that ‘ FP Sle E ARE HHH HKH HR all j lag 112 [Von. LIII (Part 1.) FIG. 4. No. 626] HOOKE’S MICROGRAPIIIA of thofe breath, nor one herefore there juices of Ve- Vi Orginiza- lefs divers Emi- is common to all Vegetables, as may appear by fos lity A i À ie cod ingesiocn ad East: t k ks which, haviog that liberty granted ray that moft Iluftrious Society, I have hereunto ad joyn'd. were made ope, pa ifcover a ‘ough concrete,and that each Ca- rate from any of the reft, without ing films, fo that I could no more blow ice,then I could through a piece of A nor with Gigs on OF conclude, that t fome of thele kind l to the contain’d fluid om ed uve has digre amd ae ge ipo ‘tis not im cr, if belp'd with better Avicro/copes,may be fo, feems with i : of fenleive Obfervations that and Dr. Clark, Thereare four Plants, two of which are little fhrub Plants, with q litle fhort ftock, about an Inch above the ground, from not le,that MICROGRAPHIA of folid ot hardned froth,or a cengeries of very {mall bubbles confolidated ftiffas well as ti ind inde ok fubfta of an Elder, with p ', yet I cannot thence ich the Sucews mutritine them; for, in juke ana by green’ fay al great fuch thofe jeer in Epele banomena of the Royal Society on account eed in pkita 116 vern, hi 29 throu Cork, But ca’ are Rail ge which Now. a themfe! not hitherto been able to it feems very of Animal whereby to but that fome in time from the tion that is inftrudt on the Humble and Senfible Plants in M" Chiffin's or pund byfician, Dottor Clar. iver this may Pi ‘den in Saint James's Park, made Auguft the 9™ 1661, Bubble, or ind of hole in the qi the | ive then curious Mr, Evelin, Dr. Henfhaw, t Members ort any other way I have nent, of ah lent ri in that form, into ti deteĝ. And that Ovfervatons Gar. Prefer, it at all h mar dis eof, fer, arbor minio pila videatur. Which e tree, fubftance, and manner of grow- > aa onion ae I bave found that the say iak Or pi hol poy Sak ay xt roy ase dake pane enag bog have much fuch a kind of MICROGRAPHIA: o barkof it, erare bark underneath remaining d the Tree, and that with other Trees) fie, dor f procs, Lape procera, Li tice detralo juvatur, é l, mira sterum Cortex iz qui r y ita confider'd, and the tree, ¢xamin'd, would, lam very the origination of Texture bye iad HUD HE EE lle aai i Suge ef EERTE $ ebay! EEE = D an i i s] pois E i E nse oak ight, and fmooth, whence are fpread feveral fticky branches, round, ftrei ah i Hil TEs, a T si pueh (PART 2.) Fica. 4. 253 254 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Vou. LIII they gave the Learned World by their justly admired Labours; he was soon taken notice of, and for his Facility in Mechanick Inventions much priz’d by them = The same Year I contriv’d and made many trials about the Art of flying in the Air, and moving very swift on the Land and Water, of which I shew’d several Designs to Dr. Wilkins then Warden of Wadham College, and at the same time made a Module, which, by the help of prings and Wings, rais’d and sustain’d itself in the Air; but fin ing by my own trials, and afterwards by Sane that the esis of a Mans Body were not sufficient to do anything considerable o that kind, I apply’d my Mind to contrive a way to make artificial Muscles; divers designs whereof I shew’d also at the same time to Dr Wilkins, but was in many of my Trials frustrated of my expectations.”? What is mentioned here of his attempts about flying, is confirm’d by several Draughts and Schemes upon Paper, of the Methods that might be attempted for that purpose, and of some contrivances for fastening suecedaneous Wings, not unlike those of Bats, to the Arms and Legs of a Man, as likewise of a Contrivance to raise him up by means of Hori- zontal Vanes plae’d a little aslope to the Wind, which being blown round, turn’d an endless Serew in the Center, which help’d to move the Wings, to be manag’d by the Person by this means rais’d aloft... . Soon after the beginning of the Royat Socriety, viz. about April 1661. a Debate arose in the Society, oceasion’d by a small Tract Printed in 1660. about the cause of the rising of Water in slender Glass Pipes, higher than in larger, and that in a certain proportion to their Bores; this Discourse was wrote and Publish’d by Hooke; the Explication of which diffieult Phenomenon made him the more regarded. The sum of his Reasonings upon this Subject he Publish’d afterward, Micrography Observ. the 6th. in which there are several very curious and then new Remarks and Hints; as to the Nature of Fluidity and Gravity, which last is farther prosecuted in his Treatise of Springs, with other excel- lent Subjects, to which the Inquisitive are referr’d for a more ample satisfaction. This, together with his former Performances, made him much re- spected by the R. Society, and on the fifth of November 1662. “ Sir Robert Moray propos’d a Person that was willing to be entertain’d as a Curator by the Society, offering to furnish them every day when! they met, with three or four considerable Experiments; which Proposition was unanimously receiv’d, Mr. Hooke being nam’d to be the Person; and accordingly the next Day of their meeting on the twelfth of November he was unanimously accepted and taken as Curator, with the Thanks of the Society order’d to Mr. Boyle for dispensing with him for their 1 From Hooke’s diary. No. 626] HOOKE’S MICROGRAPHIA 255 use, and order’d that Mr. Hooke should come and sit among them, and both bring in every Day three or four of his own Experiments, and take care of such others as should be recommended to him by the Society.” wW ORK KS | ROBERT HOOKE M.D. S.RS. Ta ra Tan DISCOUR SES, of the Tut: ROYAL SOCIETY. I. The prefent Deficiency of pppoe Foiss orxy is difcourfed of, with the Methods of rendering it more certain and hase prg nd Eksa Li red of, particularly ‘ela IIL An Hypothetical ak Mio dit WY the paame I of by the Mind in its tion ma) be coat Onan IV. An Hypothesis ication Of the caufe RANITY, Or GRA- VITATION, Magnetism, Y, Dicsures of Ean ruana e s dheir Cis and Efeis, and Hiftories of feveral ; to which are annext, -Ph ui eon ication ‘of feveral of the Fa- ‘bles in Ove 's Metamorphofes, yery.d t from other Mythologick Inter- vE Lettures for for improv: om and AstTRONOMY, with the Defcriptions of rma. Pp new pee ‘et ful cries ota Fea Contrivance! the whole full of curious Difquifitions and Tothefe Discourses is prefix the AuT Ho R’s Lire, giving an Ac- = "o Da Studies and Employments, with an a se man nF apa panera trivances and Inventions, by hii Cars ete a: rrn to the Royal Society. BLISH’D By RIC HARD WALLER, R.S. Secr. bo a: Printed by Sam Smatitand Banh Ware a ages the Royal Society) at the Premces Arms in St. Paai’s C frke bih ard. ee E a5 3 Ci tl = — dedd Ric: Daler Rey Juut kpda ` Fra. 5. From this time the Societies Journals gave sufficient: Testimonials of his Performances, all which' woul too many to particularize here, therefore I shall only touch upon some of the chief. At several Meetings of the Society in 1663, and 4. he produe’d his Microscopical Observations, and read the Explications and Discourses made upon them, which were®after publish’d in his Micrographia, at the beginning of the year 1665. p Sir John Cutler having founded a Lecture, and settl’d an Annual Stipend upon Robert Hooke, M.A. of fifty Pounds during Life (entrust- ing the President, Council and Fellows of the said Society to direct and appoint the said Mr. Hooke as to the Subject and Number of his Lee- 256 THE AMERICAN N4TURALIST [Vou. LIII tures) the Society order’d several of their Members to wait upon Sir John Cutler, with their Thanks for his particular Favour to a worthy Member, and for that — a Confidence he hath hereby exprest towards their whole Body, ete.” Fi ihis time he EERS in almost at every Meeting Experiments, Observations, Schemes of new Instruments and Inventions, or something LUP BB to the advancement of Knowledge, and very frequently read utlerian Lectures, of many whereof he publish’d, the most material a in his Tracts Printed at different times, in Quarto, call’d Lectures and Collections, &e. comprizing compendiously in one continu’d Dis- course, the chief Matters and Subjects handled in several Lectures. Thus the generous Ardor with which the Royau Society was inspir’d continued ’till the Year 1665, when, by reason of the great Mortaity then reigning, they were oblig’d to ani and break up their Weekly Meetings till the fourteenth of March 1663. . The dreadful Conflagration of a great part of the City of London House in the Strand, where, by the favour of the, then Duke of Norfolk, they prosecuted their former Inquiries, their first Meeting at Arundel House being on the ninth of Jan. 166 . n the nineteenth of Sep. 1666. he produe’d a Module he had design’d for the Rebuilding of the City, with which the Society were very well pleas’d, and Sir John Laurence the then Late Lord Major, ad- dress’d himself to the Society, expressing the present Lord Majors and Aldermens liking thereof, as also their desire that it might be shewn to his Majesty, they preferring it far before the Model drawn up by the City Surveyor. What this Model was, I cannot so well determine, but I have heard that it was design’d in it to have all the chief Streets as from Leaden- Hall corner to ies ting and the like, to lie in an exact strait Line, and all the other cross Streets turning out of them at right Angles, all the Churches, publick Buildings, Market-places, and the like, in proper and convenient places, which, no doubt, would have added much to the Beauty and Symmetry of the whole. How this came not to be accepted of I know not, but it is probable this might contribute not a little to his being taken notice off by the Magistrates of the City, and soon after made Surveyo The seas of the City, according to the Act of Parliament, re- quiring an able Person to set out the Ground to the several Proprietors, 2From the Journal of the Secretary of the Royal Society, November 9, 664. * . No. 626] HOOKE’S MICROGRAPHIA 257 Mr. Hooke was pitch’d upon, and appointed City-Surveyor for that difficult Work, which being very great, took up a large proportion of his Time, to the no small hindrance of his Philosophical Disquisitions. In this Employment he got the most part of that Estate he died pos- sessed of, as was evident by a large Iron Chest of Money found after his Death, which had been lock’d down with the Key in it, with a date of the Time, by which it appear’d to have been so shut up for above thirty Years: In this was contain’d the greatest part of what he left behind him, which was to the value of many thousands in Gold and Silver. That he might by this place justly acquire considerable Estate, I think cannot be en ag, Mr. ‘Oldenburgh, the sbi Saidiy; dying in the time of the Societies Recess, 1677. Mr. Hooke was desir’d to take his place, and take the Minutes of what considerable Matters past, which he did on the twenty fifth of October 1677. and the same day produe’d his Waterpoise and shew’d the nicety thereof. From that time he officiated in that Place, as well as his Curatorship, shewing several Experiments and Instruments in order to explain the Gravitation and Alterations in the Air by Vapours, ete. Contriving an Air-poise to shew the different specifick Gravity of the Air by a large thin ball of Glass Feryat areir Pioi this ENR he ae Nise odsbptial Chiiecoinic on Atlee in Peper-water, and other Seeds steeped in Water, confirming Monsieur Leuenhook’s Assertions, and propos’d some Improvements of Micro- scopes. Apr. 25. 1678. he shew’d an Experiment farther to explain the action of a Muscle, “ which was by a Chain of small Bladders fastened together, “so as by blowing into one Pipe, the whole might be atthe fill’d, and by that means contracted, supposing the Fibres of the Muscles which seem’d like a Necklace of Pearl in the paced ee be fill’d with a very agill Matter, which he thought most likely to be Air, which being included in so thin Skins, was easily wrought upon by Heat, Cold, or the acting Properties of the Liquors that pass between them, and so perform the lengthening and contracting of the Muscles. Aug. 1678. he read several Discourses, and shew’d Experiments in order to confirm his eis of a and springy Bodies. .. . Thus I tees ad some of his Pees A ae “It must be confessed that the latter part of his Life was nothing near so fruitful of Inventions as the former; tho’ it is certain he had a design to repeat the most part of his Experiments, and finish the Accounts, Observations 258 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Vou. LIII and Deductions from them, and had an Order for the Societies bearing the Charge thereof, in June 1696. when he propos’d likewise to perfect the Description of all the Instruments he had at any time contriv’d; but by reason of his increasing Weakness and a general Decay, he was absolutely unable to perform it, had he desir’d it never so much. Thus he liv’d a dying Life for a considerable time, being more than a Year very infirm, and such as might be eall’d Bed-rid for the greatest part, tho’ indeed he seldom all the time went to Bed. . . . being emaci- ated to the utmost, his Strength wholly worn out, he dy’d on the third of March 1703. being 67 Years, 7 Months, and 13 Days Old. His Corps was decently and handsomely interr’d in the Church of St. Hellen in London, all ‘he Members of the Royau Socrery then in Town attending his Body to the Grave, paying the Respect due to his extraor- dinary Merit. As to his Person he was but despicable, being very crooked, tho’ I have heard from himself, and others, that he was strait till about 16 Years of Age when he first grew awry, by frequent practicing with a Turn-Lath, and the like incurvating Exercises, being but of a thin weak habit of Body, which inereas’d as he grew older, so as to be very re- markable at last: This made him but low of Stature. ... He went stooping and very fast (till his weakness a few Years before his Death hindred him) having but a light Body to earry, and a great deal of Spirits and Activity, especially in his Youth. He was of an active, restless, indefatigable Genius even almost to the last, and always slept little to his Death, seldom going to Sleep till two three, or four a Clock in the Morning, and seldomer to Bed, often continuing his Studies all Night, and taking a short Nap in the Day. His Temper was Melancholy, Mistrustful and Jealous, which more in- ereas’d upon him with his Years. He was in the beginning of his being made known to the Learned, very communicative of his Philosophical Diseoveries and Inventions, till some Accidents made him to a Crime close and reserv’d. He laid the cause upon some Persons, challenging his Discoveries for their own, taking occasion from his Hints to perfect what he had not; which made him say he would suggest nothing till he had time to perfect it himself, which has been the Reason that many things are lost, which he affirm’d he knew. He had a piercing Judg- ment into the Dispositions of others, and would sometimes give shrewd Guesses and smart Characters. It must be confess’d that very many of his Inventions were never brought to the perfection they were capable of, nor put in practice till some other Person either Foreigner or of our own Nation cultivated the Invention, which, when Hooke found, it put him upon the finishing that which otherwise possibly might have lain ’till this time in its first . No. 626] HOOKE’S MICROGRAPHIA 259 Defects: Whether this mistake arose from the multiplicity of his Busi- ness which did not allow him a sufficient time, or from the fertility of his Invention which hurry’d him on, in the quest of new Entertain- ments, neglecting the former Discoveries when he was once satisfied of the feazableness and certainty of them, tho’ there wanted some small matter to render their use more practicable and general, I know not... . Whatever the answer may be, Hooke’s first and best ~ known work, the Micrographia, at once epitomizes the versatility of his genius as well as his apparent inability to see one problem through to a finish. To quote from a review of the work in the Philosophical Transactions, No. 2, Monday, April 3, 1665: The Ingenious and knowing Author of this Treatise, Mr. Robert Hook, considering with himself, of what importance a faithful History ry what portion he can, hath lately published a Specimen of his abilities in this kind of study, which certainly is very welcome to the Learned and Inquisitive world, both for the New discoveries in Nature, and the New Inventions of Art. To this end, he hath made a very curious Survey of all kinds of bodies, beginning with the Point of a Needle, and proceeding to the Microscopical view of the Edges of Rasors, Fine Lawn, Tabby, Watered Silks, Glass-canes, Glass-drops, Fiery Sparks, Fantastical Colours, Metaline Colours, the Figures of Sand, Gravel in Urine, Diamonds in Flints, Frozen Figures, the Kettering Stone, Charcoal, Wood and other Bodies petrified, the Pores of Cork, and of other substances, Vegetables growing on blighted Leaves, Blew mould and Mushroms, Sponges, and other Fibrous Bodies, Sea-weed, the Surfaces of some Leaves, the sting- ing points of a Nettle, Cowage, the Beard of a wild Oate, the seed of the Corn-violet, as also of Tyme, Poppy and Purslane. He continues to describe Hair, the scales of a Soal, the sting of a Bee, Feathers in gen- eral, and in particular those of Peacocks; the feet of Flies; & other In- sects; the Wings and Head of a Fly; the Teeth of a Snail; the Eggs of Silk-worms; the Blue Fly; a water Insect; the Tufted Gnat; a White Moth; the Shepherds-spider; the Hunting Spider, the Ant; the wan- ding. Mite; the Crab-like insect, the Book-worm, the Flea, the Louse, Mites, Vine-mites. He concludeth with taking occasion to discourse of two or three very considerable subjects, viz. - ve of the Rays of Suis in ale Air; the Fixt starrs; the Moo 260 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Vou. LIII In representing these particulars to the Readers view, the Author hath not only given proof of his singular skil in delineating all sorts of Bodies (he having drawn all the Schemes of these 60 Microscopical very noble contemplations. Here are found inquiries concerning the Propagation of Light through differing mediums; concerning Gravity; concerning the Roundness of Fruits, stones, and divers artificial bodies; concerning Springiness and Tenacity; concerning the Original of Foun- tains; concerning the dissolution of Bodies into Liquors; concerning Filtration, and the ascent of Juices in Vegetables, and the use of their Pores. Here an attempt is made of solving the strange Phenomena of Glass-drops; experiments are alleged to prove the Expansion of Glass by heat, and the Contraction of heated-Glass upon cooling; Des Cartes his Hypothesis of colours is examined: the cause of Colours, most likely to the Author, is explained: Reasons are produced, that Reflection is not necessary to produce colours, nor a double refraction: some cou- siderable Hypotheses are offered, for the explication of Light by Motion; for the producing of all colors by Refraction; for reducing all sorts of colors to two only, Yellow and Blew; for making the Air, a dissolvent of all Combustible Bodies: and for the explicating of all the regular figures of Salt, where he alleges many notable instances of the Mathe- maticks of Nature, as having even in those things which we account vile, rude and coorse, shewed abundance of curiosity and excellent Geometry and Mechanism. And here he opens a large field for in- quiries, and proposeth Models for prosecuting them He goes on to offer his thoughts about the Pores of iodine: and a kind of Valves in wood ; _ t spontaneous generation arising from the of the found in the leaf of a Nettle, how the stinging pain is created, and i e sub- contrivance and fabrick of Feathers for Flying. He delivers those par- ticulars about the Figure, parts and use of the head, feet, and wings of a y, that are not common. He observes the various wayes of the gen- erations of Insects, and discourses handsomely of the means, by which hunting Spider, and other Spiders and their Webs. And what he notes of a Flea, Louse, Mites and Vinegar-worms, cannot but exceedingly please the curious Reader. No. 626] HOOKE’S MICROGRAPHIA 261 Having dispatched these Matters, the Author offers his Thoughts for the explicating of many Phenomena of the Air, from the Inflexion, or from a Multiplicate Refraction of the rays of Light within the Body of the Atmosphere, and not from a Refraction caused by any terminating superficies of the Air above, nor from any such exactly defin’d super- ficies within the body of the Atmosphere... . e coneludeth with two Celestial Observations; whereof the one im- ports, what multitudes of Stars are discoverable by the Telescope, and the variety of their magnitudes . . . the other affords a description of a Vale in the Moon, compared with that of Hevelius and Ricciolo; where the Reader will find several curious and pleasant Annotations . . about the variations in the Moon, and its gravitating principle, Kigeuaat with the use, that may be made of this Instance of a gravity in the Moon. As to the Inventions of Art, described in this Book, the curious Reader will there find these following: 1. A Baroscope, or an Instrument to shew all the Minute Variations in the Pressure of the Air; by which he affirms, that he finds, that before and during the time of rainy weather, the Pressure of the Air is less, . Hygroscope, or an Instrument, whereby the Watery stidi, oitis in the Air, are discerned, which the Nose it self is not able to find. Which is by him full described in the Observations touching the Beard of a wild Oate, by the means whereof this Instrument is con- trived. 3. An Instrument for graduating Thermometers, to make them Stand- ards of Heat and Cold. 4. A New Engin for Grinding Optik Glasses, by means of which he hopes, that any Spherical Glasses, of what length soever, may be speedily made... . 5. A New Instrument, by which the Refraction of all kinds of Liquors may be exactly measured, thereby to give the Curious an opportunity of making Trials of that kind, to establish the Laws of Refraction. .. . Lastly, this Author despairs not that there may be found many Me- chanical Inventions, to improve our Senses of Hearing, Smelling, Tast- ing, Touching, as well as we have improved that of Seeing by Optick Glasses. Thus the ‘‘Micrographia’’ is obviously something more than ‘‘Some Physiological Descriptions of Minute Bodies made by Magnifying Glasses’’—it is a demon- stration of the advantages to be gained by the use of ar- tificial devices of precision in studying nature. The book is replete with singular anticipations of later dis- coveries and inventions by other workers and ‘‘it will hardly be deny’d that there are more excellent Philo- 262 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Vou. LIII sophical Discoveries and Hints, than in most extant of its bulk.” It contains the first study of the ‘‘ fantastical colours’? of thin plates with a partial explanation by in- terference; a theory of light as a ‘‘very short vibrating motion’’ transverse to straight lines of propagation through a ‘‘homogenous medium’’ (p. 56). Heat is stated to be ‘‘a property of a body arising from the motion or agitation of its parts” (p. 37); Fluidity is ‘‘but an effect of a very strong and quick shaking motion, whereby the parts are, as it were, loosened from each other, and consequently leave an interjacent space or vacuity” (p. 41); while ideas in regard to combustion are clearly outlined (p. 103) which foreshadow those reached by Mayow. But the biologist’s interest in the ‘‘Micrographia’’ is chiefly in Hooke’s application of his improved compound microscope (Fig. 2) to the study of animals and plants. At this time Malpighi, Grew, Leeuwenhoek and Swam- merdam were engaged in studies, with simple lenses or compound microscopes, on the secrets of the finer struc- ture of organisms which were to give them higher rank in biological history than Hooke’s desultory work in this field. Hooke, as has been said, was interested primarily in demonstrating the usefulness of his microscope and his belief that in inventions for the ‘‘improvement of the senses’’ lay the key to a more profound understand- ing of nature. This he accomplished and therefore, en- tirely aside from the other remarkable qualities of the ‘‘Micrographia,’’ the book holds a unique place in the history of biology. It paved the way, as it were, for the more special, profound and methodical studies of the contemporary founders of the morphology of organisms by creating a considerable interest in microscopy, and in addition proved to be for over a century the standard source from which writers on the microscope gleaned much information and many figures.’ 3 E. g., L. Joblot, 2d Ed., 1754; H. Baker, 1742; M. F. Ledermiiller, 1760- 1763; etc. In 1745 Baker reprinted and explained the plates of the Micro- graphia. No. 626] HOOKE’S MICROGRAPHIA 263 Among the large variety of observations made by Hooke, which are cited in Oldenburg’s review’just quoted in extenso, one in particular claims the attention of the biologist. This is ‘‘Observation XVIII. Of the Schema- tisme or Texture of Cork, and of the Cells and Pores of some other such frothy Bodies.’’ Here are clearly de- scribed for the first time the ‘‘little boxes or cells”? of or- ganic structure, and his use of the word ‘‘cell’’ is re- sponsible for its application to the protoplasmic units of modern biology. This observation, together with the plate, is presented in facsimile in Figs. 3 and 4. In Hooke’s treatise on ‘‘ The method of Improving Natural Philosophy,’’ included in the volume of his posthumous works (p. 28), this observation on cells is selected by Hooke to illustrate his method of scientific inquiry. Again, ‘‘Observation XXV. Of the stinging points and juice of Nettles, and some other Venomous Plants”? is accompanied by a figure of the lower side of a nettle leaf in which the outlines of the epidermal cells are well delineated and, as Miall remarks, ‘‘there is something very like a nucleus in one of them, but this may be acci- dental.’’ However, Hooke did not recognize any rela- tionship between the structures he observed in the nettle and in the cork. As an appendix to his gbservations on cork, the author relates some experiments on Mimosa in which he attrib- utes the ‘‘motion of this Plant upon touching ... to a constant intercourse betwixt every part of this Plant and its root, either by a circulation of its liquor, or a constant pressing of the subtiler parts of it to every extremity of the Plant’’—a partial anticipation of the modern idea of turgescence (cf. Figs. 3 and 4). The ‘‘Observation on Petrify’d wood and other Petrify’d bodies”’ is interest- ing because the author takes quite a modern point of view in regard to fossils (cf. Fig 4, Part I). And so we might continue—but as the reviewer re- marks in the Journal des Scavans, December, 1666: ‘‘ This Book contains more than can be taken notice of in an 264 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Vou. LIII Extract,’’ and we conclude the survey of this man of the past still using the words of the past: All his Errors and Blemishes, were more than made amends for, by the Greatness and Extent of his natural and acquired Parts, and more than common, if not wonderful Sagacity, in diving into the most hidden Changes, to her last and utmost Recesses; so that what Ovid said of Pythagoras may not unfittly be apply’d to him. Mente Deos adiit, et quae Natura negavit Visibus humanis, oculis ea Pectoris hausit. There needs no other Proof for this than the great number of Experi- ments he made, with the Contrivances for them, amounting to some hundreds; his new and useful Instruments and Inventions, which were numerous, his admirable Facility and Clearness, in explaining the Phe- nomena of Nature, and demonstrating his Assertions; his happy Talent in adapting Theories to the Phenomena observ’d, and contriving easy and plain, not pompous and amusing Experiments to back and prove eories; proceeding from Observations to Theories, and from Theories to farther trials, which he often asserted to be the most proper method to succeed in the interpretation of Nature. For these, his happy Qualifications, he was much respected by the most learned Philosophers both at home e from them, possibly, he might have stood in the Front. But humanum est errare. 4 Waller, op. cit. SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION. SIAMESE, AN ALBINISTIC COLOR VARIATION IN CATS COMPARATIVE studies of color inheritance in mammals have shown that pigment production throughout the group is due to similar processes and to genes probably homologous. These studies have shown, for example, that the pink-eyed albino con- dition seen in white rabbits, white rats, white mice and white guinea-pigs behaves in all cases as a simple recessive in crosses. It is probably due to variation in the same (t. e., in an homo- logous) genetic locus in all these rodents. In its usual form albinism consists in a complete absence of pigmentation from the ectoderm of the embryo and from all derivatives of that germ-layer in the adult animal. This includes, not only the hair, but also the retina and iris of the eye. Such is the condi- tion seen in the white mouse, the white rat, and the ‘‘Polish’’ r ‘‘Russian’’ rabbit. But this same locus may apparently undergo a different change which, while it behaves as the per- fect allelomorph of the pure white albino variation, differs from it in that it allows a certain amount of pigment to be produced, more particularly in the retina of the eye and in the hair at the extremities of the body (nose, ears, tail and feet). At times a small amount of pigment is formed elsewhere throughout the coat. This condition is best known in the ‘‘Himalayan’’ rabbit. Clear white albinism of the Polish rabbit is an allelomorph of Himalayan albinism. In the guinea-pig only the Himalyan type of albinism is known; in rats and mice, only the Polish type is known. In the guinea-pig, Wright has demonstrated the existence of two other albino allelomorphs, which apparently are distinct mutations of the same genetic locus. These are found in the red- eyed and in the dilute varieties described by him. Among rats Whiting and King have demonstrated the existence of a variety comparable with the dilute varieties of guinea-pigs and which they call ‘‘ruby-eyed.’’ It behaves as an allelomorph of ordi- nary albinism in crosses. ` White spotting of colored animals, sometimes called ‘‘ partial albinism,’’ is an entirely different variation, due to variation in a different locus. True albinism and spotting may by suitable crosses be made to coexist in the same individual. In this way 265 266 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LII I have frequently produced spotted Himalayan rabbits, which would show particular types of white spotting, as Dutch or English, on the feebly pigmented Himalayan background (as has also Punnett), and Wright has produced whole series of varieties of spotted red-eyed and spotted dilute guinea-pigs. Among certain rodents pink-eyed varieties occur which are due to variation in a genetic locus wholly distinct from that which is responsible for albinism. Such are the well-known pink-eyed varieties of mice having colored coats. Here the retina and the fur alike have a greatly reduced amount of black and brown pigmentation as compared with normal individuals, though yellow is unaffected. Pink-eyed rats and pink-eyed guinea-pigs are similar in appearance and in genetic behavior to pink-eyed mice. When crossed with the albino variety of the same species, they produce fully colored offspring as regards both eye and coat. The gene for pink-eye is thus seen to be complementary to the gene for albinism, with which it is known to be ‘‘linked’’ in rats and mice. Whether the two are also ‘‘linked’’ in guinea-pigs has not yet been ascertained. Among mammals other than rodents albino and pink-eyed varieties are not certainly known to occur, though white-spotted and black-eyed white varieties are common. It is-thus an open question whether the same genetic loci are found among them as among rodents. Bateson has pointed out similarities be- tween a color variety of cat, the so-called Siamese, and the Himalayan variety of rabbit. Both are born white or nearly white and later become more heavily pigmented. I may add (2) that both are inherited as recessives and (3) that in both varieties yellow pigment is largely or wholly suppressed, which is characteristic r op albino variation, but not of the pink-eye variation of roden Wright has ii that blondism among human beings (which when extreme in character is commonly known as al-. binism) is similar in nature to the albinism of rodents, being a ` graded series of allelomorphs similar to the series which he has described in the guinea-pig. It thus appears probable that the same genetic locus, which occurs in rodents and which has been called the ‘‘color factor,” occurs also in other mammals, including man. The case of the Siamese cat has seemed to me for some years deserving of more careful study. Lacking opportunity for such study myself, I sent out an inquiry several years ago through No. 626] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 267 the pet-stock journals for information about Siamese cat crosses A single reply has just come to hand, but from an authoritative source. A doctor, who prefers to remain anonymous, resident in an extensive institution in England and a fancier of Siamese cats, has employed his leisure, and the unusual opportunities afforded by his position, in studying the genetic behavior of Siamese cats in crosses with other varieties. He regards as characteristics of the Siamese breed a peculiar quality of voice and which characters often are seen in first generation crosses and so would seem to be inclined to domi- nance. But the distinetive Siamese eolor, he states, is never seen in F, individuals, ‘‘although quite a number show a mid- way color. At a glance you would say they were black, but on more careful examination you see they are near the color of the Siamese ears, seal brown. Most first crosses in my experience are black or seal, but some tortoise shell, or tortoise shell and white, or black and white.’’ These statements indicate the usual. behavior of yellow and of white-spotting in cat crosses. (See Whiting, 1918.) The Siamese color is evidently an independ- . ent character incompletely recessive in F,. The doctor contin- ues his account with a brief statement concerning a back cross of F, with pure Siamese. ‘‘I have a first cross female, black seal color, marked cross eyes, Siamese voice. She has been twice mated with a pure Siamese male. In her first litter she had two pure Siamese, perfect Siamese color. Unfortunately both died of distemper when about three months old. Her second mating resulted in one pure Siamese which is still alive. It is about five months old and is perfect in all Siamese points and fit to win [at shows]. Presumably the same sort of back-cross mat- ings as these would produce also kittens similar to the F, mother in color character, although no mention is made of them in these notes. The information given suffices to show the segregation of Siamese color as a recessive character in generations later than F,. The doctor confirms the observation of others as to the deficient pigmentation of the eye, a point of resemblance with allelomorphs of true albinism, as seen, for example, in red- eyed guinea-pigs (Castle and Wright), and in ruby-eyed rats (Whiting). He says: ‘‘The reflex which the Siamese cat shows in the dark is worth notice. It looks blood red and must be due to absence of pigment in the retina.’’ A further point of re- semblance with albinism is its distinctness from dilution as seen in ‘‘blue’’ varieties. The doctor speaks of having produced 268 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LII four Siamese which are ‘‘blue-pointed,’’ presumably as a re- sult of crosses with maltese, which are blue pigmented. An exactly similar combination I have recently secured in crossing rabbits, obtaining Himalayans with blue points in F, from a cross between ordinary black-pointed Himalayans and a self- colored rabbit which carried blue as a recessive character. To summarize, we have the following indications that Siamese coloration in cats is a form of true albinism similar to that of the Himalayan rabbit, and still more closely resembling the ruby-eyed rat and the red-eyed guinea-pig, all of which species possess also more typical forms of albinism, but which are allelo- morphs of those mentioned. (1) Siamese coloration in cats is attended by a deficiency in amount of pigmentation in both coat and eye. (2) Yellow pig- ment is more affected than black or brown pigment. (3) The pigmentation is less at birth than at a later period. (4) The character is recessive in heredity. (5) It is distinct from ‘*blue’’ dilution since it can be combined with it by suitable crosses. Siamese in cats as far as reported occurs only in a non-agouti form, as does Himalayan in rabbits bred for exhibition. But by a eross with agouti rabbits, Himalayan rabbits are obtained in F, which have agouti points. As this makes the contrast of points with body less strong, fanciers’ standards do not recog- nize the combination. Nevertheless the experiment shows agouti to be due to a genetic factor distinct from Himalayan. If Sia- mese in cats is also distinct from agouti, it may be expected that a cross of Siamese with tabby would produce Siamese tabbies in F., though the combination would probably not be pleasing to the fancier. W. E. CASTLE. BUSSEY INSTITUTION. BIBLIOGRAPHY Bateson, W. 1913. Mendel’s agea of Heredity. Castle, W. E., and S. W 1916. Studies of p in Guinea-pigs and Rats. Carnegie Insti- tution of Washington, Publication No. 241. Punnett, R. C. 1912. Inheritance of Coat-eolor in Rabbits. Journal of Genetics, 2. Whiting, P., W 1918. Inheritance of Coat-color in Cats. Journ. Exp. Zool., 25, p. 539. Whiting, P. W., and Helen Dean Kin, 1918. Biti Dilute Gray, a Third n in the Albino Series of the Rat. Jour. Exp. Zool., 26, p. 5 No. 626] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 269 THE MORPHOLOGICAL BASIS OF SOME EXPERI- MENTAL WORK WITH MAIZE OF all the plants that have been made to contribute to our knowledge of the principles of evolution and heredity in the last twenty years, probably none holds a more conspicuous place than Indian corn. The technique of its manipulation is compara- tively simple, and it exhibits an extreme variability, which is almost unique in extending to the endosperm; the behavior of a large number of its characteristics has been found amenable to a Mendelian interpretation and has aided materially in estab- lishing present-day views of heredity. Indeed, maize shares with Pisum the distinction of having been the means of the estab- lishing of Mendelism itself, for it was in connection with their work on maize that Correns and De Vries discovered Mendel’s paper. Since then its genetic behavior has been studied in detail by a number of investigators, and there is probably no other one plant that furnishes such a wealth of material illustrative of the principles of heredity. The writer has in recent years had the opportunity of examin- ing in more or less detail this same plant from the morphological point of view, and it has been found that we are far more famil- iar with the Mendelian behavior of some of its characteristics than we are with the characteristics themselves. This has led to some results illustrative of the need of very close coordination between genetics and morphology. In one of the numerous experiments made by East and Hayes, an attempt was made to interpret the Mendelian behavior of the irregularity of the rows of grains on the ear of corn. The ratios produced in the breeding experiments' were not very sig- nificant, and, after suggesting the possibility of ‘‘monohybridism with reversed dominance,’’ ‘‘fluctuating dominanee,’’ ete., they finally conclude that ‘‘it seems probable that a more complex set of conditions exists. If, as is suggested, this irregularity is similar to that in Coun- try Gentleman sweet corn, it was probably another set of con- ditions that caused the trouble. As the writer has since pointed out,’ the irregularity in the rows of this variety is the more or .Ț less complete expression of a very definite and comparatively simple state of affairs. Each female spikelet of ordinary maize 1‘*Tnheritance in Maize,’’ Bull. Conn. Agr. Expt. Sta., 167, 1911, p. 132. 2‘‘ The Morphology of the Flowers of Zea Mays,’’ Bull. Torrey Club, 43 127-144, 1916. 270 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vor. LIII produces one grain; but in Country Gentleman sweet corn, a second flower, ordinarily aborted, becomes functional, and the spikelet produces two grains. Since there is little or no com- pensation for this in the length of the cob, and insufficient differ- ence in the size and shape of the grain, the ear is producing a larger volume of embryo and endosperm than is ordinarily pro- duced in the same space. As a result of this crowded condition, the straight rows are more or less obliterated for a more econom- ical arrangement. At times, however, a set of conditions, pre- sumably environmental, may limit the size of the grain or increase the length of the cob sufficiently that the rows are almost straight, although each spikelet is still producing two grains. The genetic experiment, then, was probably dealing with an in- definite expression of a definite characteristic. If the heredity of the two-flowered condition of the spikelet had been tested, a more direct explanation would probably have been afforded. Again (p. 134), these same authorities explain the occurrence of hermaphrodite flowers upon the basis that ‘‘the immature sex organs, so-called, of maize seem endowed with the power of be- coming either stamens or carpels.’’ In so far as actual genetic results are concerned, this is, in most cases, at least, a sound working basis, but it is far from exact morphologically. There is no organ in the young maize flower that has the possibility of becoming in some cases a stamen and in others a pistil. The young flower has the ability to become either staminate or pistil- late because it contains primordia of both stamens and a pistil, one or the other of which usually does not develop to maturity.® Blaringhem’s extensive experiments,* in which he attempts to initiate mutation by means of injuries to the plant, fail to take into full account certain very significant facts of morphology. It is probably for this reason that he believes that the acquisition of hermaphrodite flowers in the maize plant is a progressive step. On the contrary, every indication points to the fact that the rudimentary stamens and pistils that have been found in the flowers of maize are the vestiges of organs that have been, and not the phylogenetic forerunners of organs that are to be. Moreover, normal behavior shows that in mutilating the plants he had merely promoted the production of suckers, which nor- mally tend to have bisexual inflorescences. Blaringhem’s method is ingenious and would, no doubt, give good results in a study of physiology of moncecism; but, the normal plant being under- 3 Ibid., pp. 129-134. 4Blaringhem, L., ‘‘ Mutation et traumatismes,’’ Paris, 1908. No. 626] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 271 stood, and full allowance being made for the recognized effects of inbreeding, it is not believed that there is any clear evidence that he produced a single new hereditary characteristic in maize. But not all of the assumptions of fundamentals upon which geneticists have based their work on maize have been so unhap- pily chosen as those cited. Most of the work that has been done on the heredity of endosperm characters depends upon the so- called ‘‘double fecundation’’ and upon the degeneracy of three of the four potential megaspores. The former of these facts was observed by Guignard® in 1901, but he did not figure it; the latter has been deduced by analogy. Circumstantial evidence was good in both eases, but evidence of this kind is not always dependable. No one would risk much in a financial way on chances like these, but some geneticists have risked years of work. In a recent paper® the writer has verified the facts as- sumed in this work. The peculiar behavior of reciprocal crosses between varieties of corn differing in the physical nature of the starchy endosperm, has been explained’? by the assumption that the two hereditary factors presumably carried by the two polar nuclei be dominant to the one factor carried by the sperm entering into the consti- tution of the primary endosperm nucleus. This idea is in accord with the multiple factor hypothesis, and the phenomenon is one of the few direct evidences that we have as to the behavior of a double application of a factor as opposed to a single application of its allelomorph. But so little is known of the morphology and the chemistry of these two kinds of starch and their relation to the surrounding tissues that it is not at all improbable that the explanation advanced may be modified by the results of further investigation. An interesting light is thrown upon the the multiple factor theory by certain other morphological peculiarities of the grain of corn. The essential idea of the multiple factor hypothesis, in a simple form, is that a single visible effect may be due to two or more factors, only one of which is necessary to produce the same effect, at least in a limited degree. Little is known of the relative natures of the two or more factors that compose the mul- tiple unit in the cases that have been investigated; they may be 5 Guignard, L., ‘‘La double fécondation dans le Mais,’’ Jour. de Bot., 15: 37-50, 1901. ®**Gametogenesis and Fecundation as the Basis of Xenia and Heredity in the Endosperm of Zea Mays,’’ Bull. Torrey Club, 46: 73-90, 1919. ' 1 Hayes, H. K., and East, E. M., ‘‘ Further Experiments on Inheritance in Maize,” Bull. Conn. igt Expt. Sta., 188, pp. 12-13, 1915. 272 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LIII alike, or they may be very different from each other. A grain of corn homozygous for yellow starch and red aleurone is dif- ferent in color from one having only one of these characteristics. But to a person with defective vision, or when viewed in a light of proper color, these two colors and a combination of the two may appear to be merely different shades of one color. By breeding this stock with a homozygous white, carrying no con- flicting factors, we should get what would be to this same defect- ive vision a perfect illustration of the behavior of multiple fac- tors. But it is in reality a case of dihybridism in which we have failed to distinguish between the two sets of allelomorphs. And who can doubt that relatively as great a lack of discrimination may characterize our chemical, physical, or morphological vision in observing some of the classical illustrations to which the mul- tiple factor hypothesis is applicable? Other examples could be selected from the work that has been done on maize, and doubtless many are available from the inves- tigations made with other plants and with animals, but these will suffice for illustration. Many of the organisms most useful for establishing and testing principles of heredity have an external appearance that may be very deceptive as an indicator of their true structure, and the true structure alone is the key to the deeper significance of their genetic behavior. AUL WEATHERWAX INDIANA UNIVERSITY ON HETEROPHYLLY IN WATER PLANTS THE occurrence of two or more different types of leaf upon one individual, which is so frequently characteristic of water plants, has long attracted the interest of botanists. The most usual case is that in which the submerged leaves are finely divided while the floating or aerial leaves are relatively simple. Lyte’s Herbal (1578) contains a vivid description of this type of heterophylly in the water buttercup. Since this description is also noteworthy for its insistence on the influence of external conditions, it may be cited here. Amongst the fleeting [floating] herbes, there is also a certayne herbe whiche some call water Lyverworte, at the rootes whereof hang very many hearie strings like rootes, the which doth Ping gui change his uppermost leaves according to the places where as it grow That whiche groweth within the water, carrieth, upon slender milki his leaves very small cut, much like the leaves of the common — mill, No. 626] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 273 but before they be under the water, and growing above about the toppe of the stalkes, it beareth small rounde leaves, somewhat dented, or un- evenly cut about. That kind whiche groweth out of the water in the borders of diches, hath none other but the small jagged leaves. That whiche groweth adjoyning to the water, and is sometimes drenched or overwhelmed with water, hath also at the top of the stalkes, small rounde leaves, but much more dented than the round leaves of that whiche groweth alwayes in the water. Among certain Nymphezacee we find a different type of hetero- phylly in which the submerged leaves are large, thin and translu- cent, somewhat resembling the seaweed Ulva. These leaves are particularly well shown in the yellow water-lily. To enumerate all the varieties of submerged leaf met with among angiosperms would be too long a task to undertake in the present paper. It must suffice to say that they arë either highly divided, ribbon-like, or else thinner and broader than the cor- responding air leaves. They are characterized anatomically by the lack of stomates and by the presence of chlorophyll in the epidermis. They are thus well suited for the absorption of car- bon dioxide in the dissolved form in which it presents itself to water plants. In considering the significance of heterophylly, it is a matter of importance to remember that the occurrence of different leaf- forms in a single individual is not confined to aquaties but occurs also in terrestrial plants. Nehemiah Grew, as long ago as 1682, pointed out that in many eases one plant bears leaves of Two Kinds or Two distinct Figures; as the Bitter-sweet, the common Little Bell, Valerian, Lady-Smocks, and others. For the Under leaves of Bitter-Sweet, are Entire; the Upper, with two Lobes: the Under Leaves of the Little Bell, like those of Pancy; the Upper, like those of Carnation, or of Sweet William. We find parallels to the heterophylly of hydrophytes, not only among terrestrial flowering plants, but also in the case of the dis- tinct ‘‘youth forms”? of conifers, and even—more remotely— in the ‘‘Chantransia’’ stage of such alge as Batrachospermum. Heterophylly is indeed so widespread that no interpretation can valid unless the condition be treated broadly as a very general attribute of plant life, rather than as a rare and exceptional phe- nomenon, for which special and individual explanations will suffice. To the earlier writers, such as Lamarck, the problem of hetero- phylly presented no difficulties. They regarded the submerged or aerial type of leaf as representing a direct response, on the 274 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vor LMI part of the plant, to the medium. The work of the last thirty years has, however, rendered this simple conception untenable ; the theory that now holds the field accords a much less prominent place to adaptation. The first observation that shook the founda- tions of the idea that leaf form necessarily depended directly on the milieu, was that of Costantin, who showed that, in the case of Sagittaria, the aquatic and aerial leaves were already differen- tiated from one another in the submerged bud; he noticed: au- ricles on a leaf which was only 2 to 3 mm. long. In Ranunculus heterophyllus, also, the leaves destined to be aerial are differen- tiated in the A large amount of experimental work has been published by various authors on the effect of conditions upon the leaf forms of heterophyllous plants, and, although some of the results are con- fused and conflicting, a study of the literature seems to justify one general conclusion—namely, that, in many cases, the sub- merged type of leaf is, in reality, the juvenile form, but can be produced later in the life history in consequence of poor condi- tions of nutrition ; the air leaf, on the other hand, is the product of the plant in full vigor and maturity. This conclusion, which is primarily due to Goebel and his pupils, is substantiated not only by experiments but by observations in the field. In many heterophyllous plants, the first leaves produced by a seedling, whether it develops on land or in water, conform, more or less, to the submerged type. This is the case for in- stance, in the Alismaceæ. In Alisma plantago, the water plantain, and Sagittaria sagittifolia, the arrowhead, the first leaves pro- duced by the seedling (or the germinating tuber) are ribbon- like, even when the young plant is terrestrial. The formation of this type of leaf can be induced again, even in maturity, by con- ditions which cause a general weakening of the plant. Costantin thirty years ago, recorded that, when the leaves of Alisma plan- tago were cut off in the process of clearing out a water course, or in a laboratory experiment, the next leaves produced were rib- bon-like, thus representing a regression to the submerged form. More recently, another worker, Wachter, tried the experiment of cutting off the roots of healthy, terrestrial plants of Sagittaria natans bearing leaves with differentiated laminæ. It was neces- sary to cut the roots away every week, as they grew again so rapidly. The result of this treatment was that the plants were found to revert to the juvenile stage, the new leaves being band shaped. When the experimenter ceased to interfere with the roots, the plants again formed leaves with lamine. Other plants, No. 626] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 275 with uninjured roots, grown as water-cultures in distilled water, also produced the juvenile leaf form, while those grown in a com- ‘plete culture solution developed their laminæ normally. The same observer recorded a case in which a plant of Hydro- cleis nymphoides Buchenau (Butomacex), which had been bear- ing the mature form of leaf, was observed to revert to the ribbon form. On examination it was found that most of the roots had died off. When a fresh crop of roots was produced, the mature type of leaf oceurred again. Another writer, Montesantos, showed by a series of experi- ments upon Limnobium Boscii (Hydrocharitacee) that, in this case also, the heterophylly is not a direct adaptation to land or water life, but that the floating leaves are ‘‘ Hemmungsbil- dungen’’ due to poor nutrition. In the water soldier, Stratiotes aloides, also, he showed that the stomatteless leaves were primary, but that their production could be induced at later stages by un- favorable conditions. An experiment tried by Goebel on Sagittaria sagittifolia indi- cated that absence of light in this case inhibits the formation of leaves of the aerial type. An observation of Gliick’s on Alisma graminifolium Ehrh., also points to the same conclusion. But it seems probable that the effect produced in these cases was not due directly to the darkness, but to the state of inadequate nu- trition brought about by the lack of light for carbon assimilation. Among the potamogetons, again, experimental work by Esen- beck has shown that reversion to juvenile leaves can be obtained under conditions of poor nutrition. For example, when a land plant of P. fluitans, which had been transferred to deep distilled water, had its adventitious roots repeatedly amputated, regres- sion was obtained to the floating type of leaf and then the sub- merged type. A similar reversion to thin narrow leaves was brought about in the case of P. natans by growing the upper internodes of a shoot as a cutting. Water lily leaves respond to experimental treatment in just the same way as the monocotyledons already mentioned. In the case of two species of Castalia, it has been found possible to in- duce the mature plants to form submerged leaves, either by re- moving the floating leaves or by cutting off the roots. This con- firms an earlier suggestion, made by an Italian writer, Arcangeli, that the development of the submerged leaves of Nymphaea lutea was due to ‘‘un indebolimento o diminuzione di energia vitale.” This suggestion has received independent, experimental confir- 276 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vorn: LIIT mation from Brand, who estimated: that a well-developed floating leaf of Nymphæa lutea was about eleven times the dry weight of a submerged leaf of the same area. Another dicotyledon, Proserpinaca palustris, which was in- vestigated by Burns, gave experimental results pointing to the same general conclusion as those already quoted. The primitive type of leaf in this plant is always a ‘‘water’’ leaf, but this type of leaf was also produced in the autumn by all the plants, re- gardless of any external conditions which the experimenter could eontrol. On the other hand, at the time of flowering and in the summer generally, almost every plant, whether growing in water or air, produced the ‘‘land’’ type of leaf—the transition from the ‘‘water’’ to the ‘‘land’’ type taking place earlier on strongly growing than on weak stems. The author considers it evident that the aquatic environment is not the cause of the division of the leaf, nor does it depend on light, temperature, gaseous con- tent of the water or contact stimulus. The only conclusion which he holds to be justified by his experiments is that Proserpinaca palustris has two forms, an adult form and a juvenile form; under good vegetative conditions, it tends to produce the adult form with the undivided leaf, the blossom and the fruit, while, if the vegetative conditions are unfavorably influenced, a reversion ean be induced to the primitive form with the submerged type of leaf. These results are consistent with those of McCallum, who had dealt with the same species at an earlier date, but his in- terpretation is slightly different. He is inclined to regard the occurrence of the water form as induced by the checking of trans- piration and the increased amount of water which hence accumu- lates in the protoplasm. This explanation is not inconsistent with the more general view that any condition tending to lower the vitality may be responsible for a reversion to the submerged type of leaf. In nature, the regression to the juvenile type of leaf sometimes occurs, not only in the case of an entire plant subjected to adverse conditions, but also in the ease of lateral shoots from an individ- ual which is otherwise producing the mature form of leaf. Goebel for instance, examined an old example of Eichhornia azurea (Pontederiacee) which had wintered as a terrestrial plant in a greenhouse; the leaves were of the mature form, differen- tiated into sheathing base, petiole and lamina, except in the case of a lateral shoot, which, on the contrary, bore the grass like, simple leaves which characterize the young plant. Goebel also No. 626] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 277 describes the occurrence of subdivided leaves of the water type on lateral shoots of normal land plants of Limnophila hetero- phylla. A corresponding reversion has been observed in the case of the side branches of plants of Proserpinaca palustris develop- ing in the air from a plant whose main stem was producing the mature type of leaf; by removing the growing apex of the stem in June, side branches of the ‘‘water type’’ were induced to de- velop. The interest of these lateral shoots, which show a reversion to an ontogenetically earlier type of leaf, is enhanced by the fact that C. and F. Darwin in ‘‘The Power of Movement in Plants”? have recorded a case of the occurrence, on lateral shoots, of leaves whose characters are probably phylogenetically earlier than those which the species normally exhibits. Their observations related to the sleep habits of the allied genera, Melilotus and Trifolium. They noticed in Melilotus Taurica that leaves arising from young shoots, produced on plants which had been cut down and kept in pots during the winter in a greenhouse, slept like those of Tri- folium, with the central leaflet simply bent upwards, while the leaves on the fully grown branches of the same plant afterwards slept according to the normal Melilotus method, in which the ter- minal leafiet rotates at night so as to present one lateral edge to the zenith. They suggest that Melilotus may be descended from a form which slept like Trifolium. The idea that the ‘‘juvenile’’ leaves produced on lateral shoots may in some cases represent an ancestral type, is consistent with the facts in the case, for instance, of the Alismaceæ, provided that the ‘‘phyllode theory’’ of the monocotyledonous leaf be accepted in the sense advocated by Professor Henslow and the present writer. According to this theory, the ancestral leaf of this family was ribbon-shaped, while the oval or sagittate blade (or ‘‘ pseudo- lamina’’) represents a later development—a mere expansion of the apex of the petiole. The submerged youth leaves of this fam- ily would thus represent a reversion to phylogenetically older orms. If the interpretation of heterophylly indicated in the present paper holds good at all widely, the teleological view of the sub- merged leaf must be considerably modified. The present writer would like to suggest that, for the old conception of heterophylly as induced by aquatic life, we should substitute the idea that such a difference between the juvenile and mature forms of leaf as would render the juvenile leaf well suited to aquatic life, has been 278 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LITI in many cases one of the necessary preliminaries to the migration from land to water, and that the aquatic angiosperms thus in- clude, by a process of sifting, those plants whose terrestrial an- cestors were endowed with a strong tendeney towards hetero- phylly. AGNES ARBER NEWNHAM COLLEGE, CAMBRIDGE COALESCENCE OF THE SHELL-PLATES IN CHITON* CHITONS are peculiar in the fact that the molluscan shell is here represented by a series of eight distinct dorsal plates, which in different genera overlap and articulate with one another to varying degrees. The full number, 8, seems, however, to be invariably present. While examining recently a series of some- what over 2,100 individuals of Chiton tuberculatus L., I came upon two cases, and two only, exhibiting any irregularity with respect to the number of the shell-plates. These were specimens, a male and a female, found near together on the beach at Cross Bay, Bermuda, in which plates 7 and 8 had in each instance almost completely fused (Figs. 2-6), so that each of these ani- mals seemed at first sight to have but 7 plates; since no rec- ords seem previously to have been made of such occurrences, they are here figured and described. In the two abnormal chitons the fused terminal plates were of similar external appearance, but in individual A, the female, the coalescence of plates 7 and 8 was somewhat less complete than in individual B, the male, as shown by the form of the inner surfaces of the compound plates. It is perhaps accidental that in both cases fusion of the respective plates is somewhat assymetrical, being more complete on the right side. As seen in Fig. 4, the muscular intersegmentum, which ordinarily re- ceives the insertion plates of the eighth valve, is represented by a relatively small tongue of tissue. 1 We owe to Dr. H. B. Guppy, F.R.S., the important idea that the habitats of plants are determined by their ‘posuere ies of structure and not vice versa. In relation to the occurrence of plants with buoyant seeds and fruits in water-side stations, he writes, ‘‘there are gathered at the mar- gins of rivers and ponds, as well as at the sea-border, most of the British plants that could be assisted in the distribution of their seeds by the agency of water. This great sifting experiment has been the work of the ages, an we here get a glimpse at Nature in the act of selecting a station * Contributions from the Bermuda Biological Station for Research, No. 104. No. 626] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 279 It may be of significance that the only instances obtained of fusions of the kind figured, occurred at a sandy beach, on the south side of Bermuda, exposed to the beating of the ocean surf. Individual A, when found, was attached to a rock, but was half-covered by sand left by the tide. Chitons in such sit- uations are frequently buried for a time beneath a foot or more of sand, and under these circumstances the over-lapping edges Outlines of valves 7 and 8 of a normal Chiton tuberculatus; a, in Ae dae hamra relations ; b, plate 8 separately. Natural size. Compound terminal plate of an abnormal C. tuberculatus (individual 4. Compound terminal plate of an ashnormeal 0. tuberculatus (individual B, 2 E cm. long; dorsal view. Natural size The same, ventral view. Natural stie. we ‘ Dorsal aspect of g aetan] end of fgg A, to show (8) reduction of intersegmentum 7-8; (a) intersegmentum 6-7. Both abnormal chitons esti- mated to be five years old. Natari size. of the shell-plates are kept tightly pressed together, thus pre- venting sand-grains from abraiding the soft inter-tegmental mantle. The posterior end of a Chiton tuberculatus is less active in turning movements, in curling-up and in similar opera- tions than is the anterior end, so that two valves, once stuck together, might, at the posterior end, have a better chance of remaining together. The incomplete union of the valves, visible when seen from their inner side, suggests that the coalesced plates started out independently. Whether or not this view be valid, it would be of interest to determine if there is any gen- eral tendency, in special localities, toward the establishment of races of chiton possessing a reduced number of plates. W. J. CROZIER DYER ISLAND, . BERMUDA 280 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LIII THE EFFECTS OF THE WINTER OF 1917-1918 ON THE OCCURRENCE OF SAGARTIA LUCIÆ VERRILL! In June, 1902, I published in the AMERICAN NATURALIST some notes on the dispersal of Sagartia lucie that tended to show that this sea-anemone had spread from the neighborhood of New Haven, Conn., along the New England coast as far north as Salem, Mass. This migration was accomplished in approxi- mately a decade, from 1892 to 1901. Since 1902 repeated ef- forts have been made to discover evidences of this species farther to the north than Salem but without avail. Apparently the species had reached its northernmost limits. Sagartia lucie was first noticed in Woods Hole, Mass., in 1898. From that year until the present it has been an extremely abundant species on the stones, mussels and eel grass in the waters of this region. On Pine Island, a narrow ridge of rocky gravel overtopped with coarse vegetation and lying in the swift tidal currents of the Hole, the narrow beaches between tides have been covered with thousands of this species of sea-anemone. When this locality was visited in June, 1918, not a single speci- men of Sagartia lucie could be found, though the particular area examined had been covered with many individuals the year before. Nor was this condition due to the relatively early date at which the search was made. Repeated attempts during low tides in July and August never yielded at Pine Island more than two or three specimens at a time, and it was quite clear that Sagartia lucie, once so prevalent in that locality, had suddenly become all but extinct there. The same was true of other situations in and about Woods Hole. In fact, a general search showed that in not a single location where this sea- anemone had been abundant in 1917 could there be found more than a paltry number of specimens in 1918 The occasion of this sudden and great diminution in the num- bers of Sagartia lucie is to be attributed, I believe, to the rigor of the winter of 1917-1918. The cold and ice of this winter were almost unprecedented. Mr. Vinal Edwards, the veteran collector of the laboratory of the United States Bureau of Fish- eries at Woods Hole, has kept a continuous record of the weather conditions of this region for a long period and this record shows, as might be expected, that the winter conditions in 1917- 1918 were more severe than for many years past. In no win- 1 Contributions from the Zoological Laboratory of the Museum of Com- parative Zoology at Harvard College. No. 626] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 281 ter during the last ten years has the sea water been at 0° C. or lower for so long a period as last winter. Beginning with the season of 1908-1909 and proceeding to that of 1917-1918, the number of days for each of the ten winters in which the tempera- ture of the seawater was O° C., or lower, was 3, 40, 44, 63, 3, 55, 0, 65, 36 and 80. Thus 1917-4918 with its 80 days of extremely cold water strikingly outruns any one of the preceding nine years. This winter was conspicuous for the formation of large amounts of anchor frost in the shallow waters about Woods Hole. This frost or ice can be seen forming on the bottom of paion bodies of salt water when the temperature of that water at 0° C., or lower. It is apparently due to the freezing of real cater that, seeping through the land, rises from the sea bottom and solidifies at once on coming in contact with sea- water below its own freezing point. This fresh-water ice is especially destructive to marine animals on the bottom and its great prevalence during the winter of 1917-1918 is probably re- sponsible for the scarcity of sea-urchins and other like forms the following summer. It probably had little or no effect on Sagartia, for this sea-anemone lives chiefly between tides and, therefore, above the level at which anchor frost is found, but as a winter phenomenon this ice is a good index of severity and it is severity in the nature of low temperature that is responsible, I believe, for the almost complete elimination of Sagartia. That this sea-anemone was not destroyed by the merely me- chanical effect of ice and waves is seen from the fact that the same stretches on Pine Island that were populated with Sagartia lucie were, and still are, covered with many specimens of Metridium marginatum. This northern species seems not to have suffered in the least from the severity of the past winter and I, therefore, conclude, since Metridium was as much ex- posed to mechanical injury as Sagartia and still survived m ordinary numbers, that Sagartia succumbed to low temperature rather than to any other factor in its environment. This is in accord with the general belief, originally expressed by Verrill, that Sagartia lucie is a southern species introduced by some accident into northern waters. Granting this conclusion, lit is easy to understand why this species has not migrated farther northward into colder waters and why in severe winters it is almost exterminated in localities such as Woods Hole. G. H. PARKER HARVARD UNIVERSITY 282 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou LIII TAXONOMY AND EVOLUTION A REJOINDER THE writer has great sympathy with much of what ‘‘X’’ has to say on the above subject in a recent number of the AMERICAN NaTuRALIST (Vol. xuvm, 369-382).! Needless to say, however, he can not agree with all. True there is much in systematic zoology that is slipshod, but till statistics can be produced to show that the percentage of slipshod work produced by sys- tematice zoologists is higher than in other fields of zoology, the writer of this article has a temporary residence in Missouri. He is of the opinion, also, that as great a percentage of the work of the systematic zoologist will stand the test of time as the work of the anatomist or any other worker in the field of zoology and proposes to remain of that opinion until time, the great leveler, proves to the contrary. Linnzus is apparently not the only genius that has left the back door open and that has ‘‘been followed by a crowd of other workers eager to attain to immortality,” as witness the great mass of half-digested literature on genetics, say, that has been crowded into the past ten or a.dozen years. It would be a sad state of affairs indeed if systematics as a whole were not im- proving. That there have been occasional backward steps there is no doubt, but on the whole the progress has been forward. I hardly believe that even the systematists are as big fools as ‘*X”’ pictures them to be, for I have yet to discover in my rambles a systematist who believed that his work was final. Heaven forbid. The ezar in zoological nomenclature may arise and issue his fiat, but there will be later czars who will do away with them. For surely ‘‘X’’ would not have us believe that the day will ever dawn in this world when all things are settled. My shorter catechism is somewhat awry, but surely such a happy state is reserved for the Great Beyond. Without wishing to disparage the modern workers T wish to say that some of the older workers did write ‘‘careful descrip- tions,’’ as witness the following case which has been called to my 1The present paper was written in July, 1914, soon after reading the r by ‘*X.’’ It was laid away but now that it is more than four years old ‘‘ going on’’ five, as children say, it seems best to submit it for publi- eation. No. 625] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 283 attention. One of the early systematic entomologists described a species, on external characters only, in about three lines. Later entomologists were puzzled because the species had charac- ters common to two widely separated genera; and one systematist said it belonged to one genus, and another said to a widely divergent genus, while a third said it was simply another name for a common form. Yet, behold, when the species was redis- covered it was found to belong to a new genus with characters common to the two widely divergent genera. Now, what’s the answer, certainly the original description must have been a good one otherwise how could workers nearly a century later rec- ognize the characters? Isolated quotations from descriptions of any species look ridiculous (p. 370), but no more so than isolated quotations from the work of sedli neurologist or what not. A kindly feeling for my fellow workers in other fields and for the editor of the AMERICAN NATURALIST stays me from quoting at length and verbatim. Fortunately ‘‘X’’ has sufficiently concealed his identity so that I can not quote some of his own discussions until he yawns. Neither is my soul more deeply stirred by con- templating the poor hymenopterist, squinting at his box of dried ‘‘bugs’’ stuck on pins; than it is by the poor hunch-backed short- sighted cytologist (let us say) who, peering through his high power compound microscope, imagines that the world is circum- scribed by his field of view and that a cell, or a nucleus, or a chromosome, is all there is to zoology. **X’’ seems to deplore the fact of specialization in zoology and at the same time seems to ignore the fact that it is along these lines that the world moves. Why should we not have neurolo- gists, taxonomists, hemipterists, ete., in zoology just as we have masons, carpenters, roofers, pinata, tinners, ete. How many railroads would have been built in this world or how much progress would have been made in any other line of human en- deavor if every man had to be a jack-of-all-trades? Do we hire a man to build us a house? Most certainly not. We hire a brick mason to lay the foundation, a carpenter to erect the frame, another one to put on the weather boarding, and still another to do the finishing inside; and so on until our house is finished and the whole structure stands only as long as the work of each one of these individual workers will stand. So it seems to me it is in zoology, the systematist lays the foundation upon 284 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vor. LIII which the whole structure is raised. And while the whole method of systematice zoology is open to criticism by anatomists, or what not, yet a certain amount of systematic work must be done before the anatomist can develop his work. If we take this position it seems to me that we must grant that the sys- tematist must be far to the forefront, well in advance of the workers in other fields. And certainly this much must be said in his favor that he has turned out enough ‘‘new species’’ in the last few years to keep the rest of the zoologists busy for a year or two. ‘*X’s’’ whole attitude is that the systematist makes mistakes and that he sticks only to external characters. In regard to the first I would call ‘‘X’s’’ attention to the fact that anatomists a little less than 300 years ago believed the arteries carried air, not blood. And it seems to me if we go back about 250 years we find one Robert Hooke describing ‘‘little boxes (empty) of cells dis- tinct one from another’’; and wasn’t it only about half a century ago that the cytologist awakened to the fact that the boxes were not as empty as might seem? Now the question to my mind is this, would we know as much about cytology as we know to-day, if Hooke had not discovered his empty boxes? I think not. And as a necessary corollary would we know as much about the animal world as we now know if systematists had not described new species? I think not. The fundamental basis of systematic work, it seems to me, must always be external characters, though they may be variable and unsatisfactory in many respects. What we all want and what I believe all systematists are striving for though some of their strivings may be misdirected is, among other things, ease of identification which, to my mind, implies reference to external characters. I, see a woodpecker sitting in a tree and identify him as a yellow-bellied sapsucker by the fact that he has, among other characters, a white stripe down his wings. Very unscientific, I grant, but highly satisfactory to me if I am collecting not sapsuckers but downy woodpeckers. Also to the sapsucker if the alternate character which enabled me to identify him was the presence of extra small convolutions on his cerebellum, I make my plea for systematic zoology as systematic zoology, not for its ‘‘phylogenetie classification of animals,’’ nor for its on geographical distribution, variation or heredity or sarsii else. The description of ‘‘560 new species of Zonitide’”’ No. 625] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 285 may not seem soul-inspiring work to ‘‘X,’’ but to the deseriber it may have been exceedingly so. The description of 560 new species of Zonitide makes it possible for some student of va- riation or of ‘‘ phylogenetic classification’’ to work on the Zonitidee in a way that would not have been possible if these 560 ‘‘new species’’ had not been described, and no one man would have been able to describe the 560 new species and work their em- bryology, internal anatomy, neurology, ecology, geographical dis- tribulation, behavior, variation, mendelian relations, etc., and live to tell the tale. Furthermore, if there is any man that has the aptitude to describe ‘‘560 new species of Zonitidse’’ my benediction is ‘‘let him go to it.’’ And while 585 of his ‘‘560 new species’? may prove to be false alarms that have never been turned in, at the same time it does not seem to have occurred to ‘‘X’’ that he may be doing much less harm thusly employed than if, he were rampant with scissors, scalpels and needles or with killing agents, stains and a microtome trying to discover the true in- wardness of the Zonitide. I do not want to be misjudged by any one who may think that I am making a plea for slipshod work, but I do want to make a plea for the isolated worker who is plodding away in his own particular field without hope of reward or recompense in this day or generation. Let us be very careful about setting our stamp upon a thing as worth while or not worth while. Mendel, the poor isolated monk, working away with his peas, never dared dream, I venture to say, that his work would revolutionize the biological thought of the twentieth century. Thus ‘‘X’’ may have the misfortune to view in a future reincarnation the sad spectacle of the zoologists of say 200 years hence loudly acclaiming the good work of the describer of the 560 new species of Zonitide, while at the same time they point with scorn to the work of the anatomist who discovered (?) that the digestive system of the Zonitide runs up hill. The writer has the fortune or misfortune, as pleases your point of view, to be the entomologist of a state experiment station. His principal duties as entomologist are the intensive studies of two widely separated species of extremely injurious insects. This work is carried on under the Adams Fund by grants from the United States Department of Agriculture. Both projects were so outlined as to involve everything about these 286 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII two insects that could be discovered by the writer; internal and external anatomy, embryology, life history, parasites, ete. Present indications are that it will take an average of about six years to finish (?) each one of these projects. Yet such a seemingly slow rate of progress is made possible only by the fact that some one working somewhere has described these two species and given them names. The one species was described without the describer ever having seen the male! Yet without this inadequate description progress on this problem would have been very greatly delayed. And so it is in every other field that these problems touch. Some one has described some- where 29 species of parasitic hymenoptera, one of this number preying upon one of the species involved. Yet the describer knew only the adult and that only imperfectly, but his knowl- edge plus my own sends us one step nearer the complete knowl- edge of this species which ‘‘X’’ craves. And our knowledge of this species plus some one’s knowledge of other related species raises us just one step nearer the truth which should be the goal of all human thought, and all science, zoology not even excepted. _ I am interested in the phylogenetic relationship of a group of insects of no great economic importance. Especially am I interested in the genealogical tree of these insects as shown by the groups of characters of one structure. Now such work is made possible because three men in this country have devoted their entire time describing new species and new genera in this group. Without these descriptions many of which might have served as well as the one quoted on page 370, and without the collections of insects which these three men have made it would be impossible for me to make any progress along the line of a genealogical tree, which it is my fond hope will be of some use to the systematists of this group and to zoologists in general, I have long wondered what could be called trivial characters. A few illustrations of the importance of so-called trivial char- acters in other fields than systematics may perhaps occur to ‘‘ X.” One of the most important that has come to my notice was that of a cytologist who discovered differences in the chromosomal characters of two different sets of individuals of the ‘‘same species’’ only to discover later that systematists had long dis- tinguished between these two forms on the basis of characters more trivial than whether they were ‘‘pink with blue spots” or No. 625] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 287 ‘“blue with pink spots.’’ Again two species of scale insects are separated by the fact that one has the median lobes of the pygidium rounded while the other has the median lobes conical (external characters). Yet one lives on oak trees and has at least four generations annually and the other lives on maple trees and has only a single generation annually. Now if ‘‘X’’ thinks that these facts would have been discovered as easily and as quickly as they have been discovered, if Professor Comstock had not pointed out these ‘‘trivial characters’’ some thirty-odd years ago, he thinks differently than I think. Yet the application of these facts is of vast importance to the horticulturalist and land- scape architect or any other artisan who works to beautify our landscapes with trees, or any one who attempts to control these two pests. I have no doubt that Linneus was accused of relying on trivial characters for separating some of his genera and species. It would be interesting if history could tell us and it would be still more interesting if we could look into the future, say 100 years, and see what systematists and others will say about the present-day systematists who overlooked such perfectly obvious characters as the extra spines on the hind leg of species ‘‘a’’ contrasted with ‘‘b’’ and their wonder and amazement that systematists of this our glorious twentieth century should have overlooked such important and obvious characters. So it will be in other fields. The histologist of the future will wonder why we used such crude killing and fixing agents; and will, more than likely, refer to our finest precision microtomes with a shrug much as we refer to the stone hatchets of the men of the Old Stone Age. | I make this somewhat extended plea because it seems to me that ‘‘X’’ has unconsciously done the systematists a great wrong.* ‘‘X’s’’ attitude may discourage promising young men from entering the field of systematics where their help is greatly needed. Let us therefore lay aside our critical air and our sitting in judgment to decide just what is worth while and what isn’t and turn our attention to utilizing the results of other workers in other fields to the greatest extent. The systematist 2 That all may see that my plea is entirely unselfish, I will state that I am not a systematist and that I have never described a single ‘‘new spe- cies.’’? My attitude is simply one of gratitude to the systematists who have helped me with my problems. 288 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von LIH is human like the rest of us, he has his limitations like the rest of us, but he believes, I think justly, that his work is pioneer work of great importance; and, if occasionally he gets beyond the limited range of our embryologist’s microscope or our an- atomist’s scalpels and needles, let us not accuse him of wandering along the River of Doubt or being a lineal descendant of the famous Baron Munchausen. But let us look upon the sys- tematist’s work as the foundation for the glorious structure, modern zoology, which completed by his other co-workers will stand four square to the wind for all time to come. We do not need to defend systematics on the basis of ‘‘(1) the advertise- ment theory; (2) the recognition mark theory ;’’ although both are perhaps more important than ‘‘X’’ intimates. But what is vastly more important is the fact that systematics is the basis for all real work in zoology. And the morphologist or anatomist who takes the attitude that systematics is to be entirely avoided? or, what is worse, is to be simply laughed at is placing himself in the same class as the man who says that there is no such thing as matter in the world. Sooner or later he is going to bump into the fact that systematics must play its part in his field and that systematics is broader than the question whether the ‘‘second joint is longer than the third’’ or whether a species should be called aabus Smith or beabus Jones. Z. 3 Just what does ‘‘X’’ think about the anatomist who discussed at great length the anatomy of, let us say, the ‘‘ American frog (Rana temporaria) ’’ because that was the name given the frog in his perfectly good English ‘*Text-book of Zoology’’; when the context shows that the frog he was dealing with was the common leopard frog (Rana pipiens)? THE AMERICAN NATURALIST VoL. LIII. July-August, 1919 No. 627 ON THE USE OF THE SUCKING-FISH FOR CATCHING FISH AND TURTLES: STUDIES IN ECHENEIS OR REMORA, II. E. W. GUDGER AMERICAN MUSEUM OF NATURAL History, New York Crry THe FısaerMmaNn-Fısau 1x MozaMmBIQUE WATERS Ix the year 1884, Mr. Frederick Holmwood, British Consul at Zanzibar, by publishing an article in the Pro- ceedings of the Zoological Society of London, brought this extraordinary use of this remarkable fish to the at- tention of the scientific world. Chancing on this article, I became greatly interested in the matter and have been led to collect all the available data and to present it herein to those who may be interested. On a trip in a steam launch from Pemba to Zanzibar, Holmwood had his attention called to a number of remo- ras which were attached to the sides and bottom of the boat. To these the natives on board gave the name **Chazo.’’ Later at Zanzibar he saw natives digging out diminutive canoes, too small for any normal use, which he was told were for the ‘‘Chaza’’ (so he under- stood the native word). Now ‘‘Chaza’’ is the word for oysters or other bivalves, hence- he thought that these were used to gather such in, but his servant told him that it was a ‘‘house’’ for the ‘‘Chazo’’ or sucking-fish kept by most fishermen in their huts. Later he learned that the native fishermen use the Chazo fish to catch turtles and large fish of any kind. And later still in 289 290 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII Madagascar he was informed that sharks and even large crocodiles were caught by the use of a fish called Tarundu' which was trained for the purpose. Unfor- tunately, just here Holmwood gave vent to his ineredu- lity and his informants being greatly incensed refused to talk with him further on this matter. Holmwood spent considerable time in gaining the con- fidence of the native fishermen of Zanzibar and was re- warded by being allowed to visit their huts and examine the ‘‘Chazo.’’? These he found to be remoras (echeneis?) from 2 to 4.5 feet long and from 2 to 8 pounds in weight. They were kept in the little canoes in the cabins and were so tame as readily to come to the surface of the water at the appearance of their masters, by whom they allowed themselves to be freely handled. Each Chazo had a strong iron ring or loop fixed just above the tail [text-figure 1] for the purpose of attaching a line to when being em- TEXT-FiGURD 1. Tail of sucker-fish with band and-ring. (After Holmwood, 1884.) ployed in hunting. In some cases these appendages had evidently re- mained on for years, during which the fish had so grown that the iron had become imbedded in a thick fleshy formation. In two instances the ring had been inserted in the muscular substance at the root of the tail [fin], but generally a simple iron band was welded around the thinnest part of the body a few inches from the tail, which kept it from slipping off. To this was riveted a small movable ring or loop resembling that of a watch-handle. In one ease [text-figure 2] this loop was fastened on by servings of brass wire in a similar manner to the rings of a fishing rod. 1 Every effort has been made to trace down the use of the Tarundu, but books on the fishes of Madagascar are few, and none of them nor the works of travel consulted have given any clue. No. 627] STUDIES IN ECHENEIS OR REMORA 291 Holmwood purchased one of these fish to send to Eng- land but it was killed by a crane. A second one died, probably from lack of a fresh supply of water. He after- wards arranged to buy another on its return from a fish- ing trip. It was brought to me a few weeks later minus its ring, and with a large wound or rent above the tail, part of which was gone. The owner declared that it had caught two turtles, which he showed me lying in his canoe, and that it had afterwards affixed itself to a large shark and, holding on after all the spare line had been paid out, the tail had given TEXT-FIGURE 2, Tail of sucker-fish with loop and servings. (After Holmwood, 1884.) way. He stated that the Chazo had then relinquished its hold and re- turned in its mutilated state to the boat. He assured me that this was not an unusual oceurrence and that after a time a fresh ring would be attached and the fish become as useful as before. I endeavored to pre- serve one of these Chazos in spirits of wine, but failed owing to the inferior quality of the spirit. This specimen measured 2 feet 8 inches in length and weighed 314 pounds. The sucker contained 23 pairs of lamelle. Holmwood wanted to go out with the fishermen and see the fishes at work. But as the distance to the fishing- grounds was considerable, as the trips lasted fifteen days, and as the boats were small and lacked accommodations for a European, he was forced to desist. Thus he failed to become an eye witness to this remarkable procedure.? 2 Under date of 1883, a writer signing himself Phil. Robinson published a pamphlet entitled ‘‘ Fishes of Fancy—Their Place in Myth, Fable, Fairy Folk-Lore.’? This was issued as a hand book for the great International Fisheries Exhibit of that year in London. In this is a ver- batim quotation from an article by Holmwood on the use of the fisherman fish in the official catalogue of the exhibition. After much difficulty this official catalogue was located and in it was found Holmwood’s origi 292 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LIII Holmwood’s interesting account is however not the first for the use of the living fish-hook in Mozambique waters. In the year 1829 Lacépède published his ‘* His- toire Naturelle des Poissons,’’ in which, with reference to foreign fishes, he largely made use of the manuscripts of the lamented naturalist, Commerson. On page 490 of Tome III we read: Commerson ... has written that this fish (Hcheneis naucrates) fre- quents very often the coast of Mozambique, and that near to this coast it is employed for fishing for marine turtles in a very remarkable manner, due to the power which the Echeneis possesses of sticking to them. We think that we ought to report here the data which Commer- son has collected on this subject so very curious, the only of the kind which has ever been observed. [ ?] There is attached to the tail of the living Naucrates a ring of diameter sufficiently large not to incommode the fish, and small enough to be re- tained by the caudal fin. A very long cord is attached to this ring. When the Echeneis has been thus prepared, it is placed in a vessel full of salt water, which is renewed very often, and then the fishermen place this in their boats. They then sail towards those regions frequented by marine turtles. These animals have the habit of sleeping at the surface of the water on which they float, and their sleep is so light that the least noise of an approaching fishing-boat is sufficient to wake them and cause them to flee to great distances or to plunge to great depths. But behold the snare which they set from afar for the first turtle which they perceive asleep. They put into the sea the Naucrates furnished with its long cord. The animal, delivered in part from its captivity, seeks to escape by swimming in all directions. There is paid out to it a length of cord equal to the distance which separates the sea turtle from the boat of the fishermen. The Naucrates retained-by the line, makes at first new efforts to get away from the hand which masters it. Soon, however, perceiving that its efforts are in vain, and that it cannot free itself, it travels around the circle of which its cord is some fashion a radius, in order to meet with some point of adhesion and consequently to find rest. It finds this asylum under the plastron of the floating turtle, to which it attaches itself easily by means of its buekler, and account. He wrote up for this an account of the fisheries - Zanzibar and TERE by giving a short description of fishing with the ‘‘chazo.’’ This n very abbreviated form the data included above, and ends with the natant ‘*T hope to forward a specimen of this interesting fish before the close = exhibition.’’ However, as indicated previously he was unable to do No. 627] STUDIES IN ECHENEIS OR REMORA 293 gives thus to the fisherman, to whom it serves as a fulerum, the means of drawing to them the turtle by pulling in the cord.® This account of Commerson-Lacépéde’s is very circum- stantial and exceedingly interesting, but it is not the first account of the fisherman fish, and not even the first for East African waters, for in 1809 and 1810 Henry Salt under orders of the British government made a voyage to Abyssinia by way of the Cape and the Mozambique Channel, stopping at Masuril, a village on the harbor of Mozambique. Of this visit he says under date of Sep- tember 9, 1809 (his book was published in 1814): As he [the Bishop of Masuril] was aware of my wish to collect the rarities of the place, he made me a present . . . of a large sucking-fish (Echeneis naucrates) . . . which had just been brought in by a fisher- man. All the Portuguese gentlemen, whom I conversed with on the subject, agreed in assuring me that fish of this kind were employed on the coast in catching turtles. The mode of doing this is by confining the fish with a line to the boat, when it is said invariably to dart forwards, and to attach itself by its sucker to the lower shell of the first turtle found in the water, which prevents its sinking, and enables the fisher- man to secure his prey. The reason for the fish fastening on to the turtle is supposed to be done (as the Bishop observed) with a view to self-preservation, and its strength is so great that, when once fastened, the turtle is rarely known to escape. Earlier still (in the latter half of the eighteenth cen- tury) a Swede named Andrew Sparrman made a voyage to the Cape of Good Hope, and in that part of his book dealing with the land of Natal, in the French translation published at Paris in 1787 he wrote: They [the inhabitants of the country] carry on a very singular method of fishing for turtles. They take alive a fish called Remora, and fixing two cords, one to its head and the other to its tail, they then throw it into the depths of the sea in the region where they judge that there ought to be turtles, and when they perceive that the animal has attached itself to a turtle, which it soon does, they draw in to them the Remora and with it the turtle. It is said that this manner of fishing is also carried on in Madagascar. The same account in brief form is found on pages 170-171 of Pasfield Oliver’s life of Commerson (1909). 294 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII This account is not found in the English. translation of Sparrman’s voyage, and I have not had access to the original Swedish edition, but it is found in the French edition in a sort of appendix to that section describing South Africa and is credited to Middleton’s ‘‘Geog- raphy.’’ Inspection of volume I (1777) of. this latter work revealed the account substantially as given above, but in quotation marks with no hint whatever of its ulti- mate source. Humboldt (1826) refers to a similar incident related by Captains Dampier and Rogers. Dampier was worked through twice without finding the reference, but a third going through his ‘‘ Voyages’’ page by page revealed it as an annex to part 3 of his volume III, ‘‘A Discourse of Winds,’’ etc. (6th edition, 1729). Middleton has copied it almost word for word, so it need not be repeated here. It will be of interest, however, to note that Dampier says that this ‘‘annexed paper’’ was ‘‘received from my inge- nious Friend, Capt. Rogers, who is lately gone to that place (‘Natal in Africk’): and hath been there several times before.’”* It must be remembered that Holmwood wrote of a fish called Tarundu used in Madagascar as a living fish hook, and Lacépéde quotes Commerson that a sucking fish is so used in the Isle of France as well as in the Mozambique country and lastly that Dampier quotes Rogers as to this use also in Madagascar. Acting on these hints a good deal of time has been spent in hunting for such accounts not only in books on the fishes of these islands but also in books of travel and at this writing three corroboratory accounts have been found. The first is to be found in Pollen’s work on the fisheries of Madagascar (1874). 4 The index to Rogers’ book (‘‘A Cruising Voyage around the World,’’ 1726) does not contain the words echeneis, remora or sucking-fish. Careful examination of the book, and a minute inspection of that part relating to South Africa, gave no results whatever. Dampier’s ‘‘ Voyages’’ show that he was keenly observant of natural history objects wherever he went, while Rogers paid little or no attention to such matters. It seems likely that the | foregoing account was communicated to Dampier by word of mouth or by letter from Rogers. No. 627] STUDIES IN ECHENEIS OR REMORA 295 For Malagassy waters he quotes the use of Hcheneis as given by Middleton, Commerson-Lacépéde and Salt, an for other waters other authors to be referred to later. He is not clear as to its use in his own time but he seems to indicate that in his day it was so used. ` Our next reference is dated 1897. In the Antananarivo Annual for that date (published by the London Mis- sionary Society at the capital of Madagascar) there is under ‘‘Natural History Notes’’ a translation by James Wills of a native manuscript which reads as follows: In the sea off the northwest coast of Madagascar a fish is found called by the people Hamby. It is round and long, somewhat like a lizard, but its tail unfolds for swimming like that of a gold-fish, and it has fins on each side. The length of a full-sized one is about that of a man’s arm, and its girth about that of his thigh. Its back fin, from about one quarter of its length up to its head, is just like a brush, and it has a liquid about it, sticky like gum, and when it fastens onto a fish from below with this brush on its head the fish cannot get away, but is held fast. On account of this peculiarity of the Hamby, the people of Sambirano use it to fish with. When they catch one they confine it in a cage of light wood, which they fasten in the sea, and.feed the fish daily with cooked rice, or cassava, or small fish; and when they want to use it, they tie a long string round its tail and let it go, following it in a canoe. When it fastens on a fish they pull it in and secure the spoil. There is a sea-turtle called by the people Fanéhana,® which the Hamby is fond of catching, and this the people prize on — of the shell, which is of commercial value. The above account is given almost word for word by James Sibree in his book ‘‘ A Naturalist in Madagascar,” 1915. Sibree, whose experiences in Madagascar cover a period of fifty years, and who as his book shows was a very close observer, evidently believed in this use of the sh. Tuer Huntine-FisH or THE West INDIES However, the accounts quoted of the remarkable use of the Remora as a hunting fish in the Mozambique coun- try are not the first that we have of such employment. For the very beginning we must go back to the second 5 This is probably the tortoise-shell turtle. 296 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. LIII voyage of Columbus to the New World in 1494. This account given below is to be found in the writings of Peter Martyr d’Anghera, who was a prominent figure at the court of Ferdinand and Isabella and the foremost letter writer of his day. In 1511 Martyr published at Seville nine books and part of the tenth of his Decade I, the Decade of the Ocean, one of the component parts of his ‘‘De Orbe Novo,’’ which has since appeared in many editions and translations. Possibly the best translation available for the general reader is MacNutt’s, published by Putnam in 1912, but as better preserving the spirit of the times, I pricy to give Richard Eden’s translation made in 1555, the quaint English of which reads as fol- lows: At the Ides of Maye, the watche men lokinge owte of the toppe castell of the shyppe towarde the Southe, sawe a multitude of Tlandes standinge thick together, beynge all well replenished with trees, grasse, and herbes, and wel inhabyted. In the shore of the continent, he [Columbus] chauneed into a nauigable ryver whose water was soo hotte, that no man myght endure to abyde his hande therein any tyme. The daye followinge, espyinge a farre off a canoa of fyshermen of th(e) inhabitants, fearinge least they shulde flye at the syght of owre men, he commaunded certyne to assayle them pryuily with the shyppe boates. But they fearinge nothinge, taryed the comminge of owre men. Nowe shal you heare a newe kind of fyshinge. Lyke as we with greyhoundes doo hunt hares, in the playne fieldes so doo they as it were with a huntyng fysshe, take other fysshes. This fysshe was of shape or fourme vnknowen vnto vs: but the body thereof, not muche vnlyke a greate yele: havinge on the hynder parte of the heade, a very towgh skynne, lyke vnto a greate bagge or purse. This fysshe is tyed by the syde of the boate with a corde litte downe soo farre into the water, that the fysshe maye lye close hyd by the keele or bottom of the same, for shee may in no ease abyde the sight of the ayer. Thus when they espie any greate fysshe, or tortoyse (whereof there is great abundance bygger then great targettes) they let the corde at lengthe. But when she feeleth her selfe loosed, she enuadeth the fysshe or tortoyse as swiftly as an arrowe. And where she hath once fastened her howld she casteth the purse of skynne whereof we spoke before; And by drawyng the same togyther, so graspeleth her pray, that no mans strength is sufficient to vnloose the same, excepte by lyttle and lyttle drawinge the lyne, shee bee lyfted sumwhat above the brymme of the water. For then, as sone as she seeth the brightness of the ayer, she lettethe goo No. 627] STUDIES IN ECHENEIS OR REMORA 297 her howlde. The praye therefore, beinge nowe drawen nere to the brymme of the water, there leapeth soodenly owte of the boate into the sea soo manye fysshers, as maye suffice to holde faste the praye, vntyll the reste of the coompany haue taken it into the boate. Which thinge doone, they loose so muche of the cord, that the hunting fysshe, may ageyne returne to her place within the water: where by an other corde, they let downe to her a piece of the praye, as we use to rewarde grey- houndes after they have kylled theyr game. This fysshe, they caule Guaicanum, but owre men caule it Reuersum. They gave owre men foure tortoyses taken by this meanes: And those of such byggenes that they almoste fylled theyr fysshinge boate. For these fysshes are esteemed amonge them for delicate meate. Owre men recompensed them ageyne with other rewardes, and soo let them departe.® Curiously enough a repetition of this story by Martyr. himself has been completely overlooked by all who have had oceasion to refer to his Reversus story. I myself did. not find it until, some two years after making notes and copying his account as quoted above from Eden, I chanced to go over the ‘‘Decades’’ again page by page and stumbled on it. Since Martyr himself has not been quoted directly it will be of interest to give this second account from MacNutt’s excellent translation of Decade VIII, Book 8, pages 299-300. Let us now consider the hunting fish. This fish formerly vexed me somewhat. In my first Decades, addressed to Cardinal Ascanio, I stated amongst other marvels, if I remember properly, that the natives had a fish which was trained to hunt other fish just as we use quadrupeds for hunting other quadrupeds, or birds for hunting other birds. So are the natives accustomed to catch fish by means of other fish. Many people, given to detraction, ridiculed me at Rome in the time of Pope Leo for citing this and other facts. It was only when Giovanni Rufo di Forli, Archbishop of Cosenza, who was informed of all I wrote, returned to Rome after fourteen years’ absence as legate of Popes Julius and Leo in Spain, stopped the mouths of many mockers, and restored me my reputation for veroai In the beginning I alab could hardly believe the story, but I received my information from trustworthy men whom I ers. Everybody has assured me that they have seen fishermen use this fish just as commonly as we chase hares with French dogs, or pursue the wild deer with Molossians. They say that this fish makes good eating. ® This is a literal copy of Arber’s literal copy of Eden, save that the old-fashioned {-shaped s has had to be replaced by the modern letter 298 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LII It is shaped like an eel, and is no larger. It attacks fish larger than itself, or turtles larger than a shield; it resembles a weasel seizing a pigeon or still larger animal by its throat, and never leaving go until it is dead. Fishermen tie this fish to the side of their barque, holding it with a slender cord. The fish lies at the bottom of the barque, for it ` must not be exposed to the bright sun, from which it shrinks. The most extraordinary thing is that it has at the back of its head a sort of very tough pocket. As soon as the fisherman sees any fish hi little cord. Like a dog freed from its leash, the fish descends on its prey and turning its head throws the skin pouch over the neck of the victim, if it is a large fish. On the contrary, if it is a turtle, the fish attaches itself to the place where the turtle protrudes from its shell, and never lets go till the fisherman pulls it with the little cord to the side of the barque. If a large fish has been caught (and the fishermen do not trouble about the small ones), the fishermen fasten stout cords to it and ' pull it into the air, and at that moment the hunting-fish lets go of its prey. If, on the contrary, a turtle has been caught, the fishermen spring into the sea and raise the animal on their shoulders to within reach of their companions. When the prey is in the barque, the hunt- ing-fish returns to its place and never moves, save when they give it a piece of the animal, just as one gives a bit of quail to a faleon: or until they turn it loose after another fish. I have elsewhere spoken at length concerning the method of training it.” The Spaniards call this fish Reverso, meaning one who turns round, because it is when turning that it attacks and seizes the prey with its pocket-shaped skin. This remarkable story of Martyr’s has been repeated by many writers from his day almost to this and espe- cially by the Spanish chroniclers of the early political and natural history of the West Indies. Many of these, however, add to the original story certain details which will be of interest to include herein. The first of these is the historian Oviedo, whose ‘*‘Sumario’’ was published but five years (1516) after Martyr’s ‘‘Decades of the Ocean,’’ and whose ‘‘Chron- icles’’ were first published in 1535. My excerpt is taken from the Salamanca edition of 1547, but there is no rea- son to think that this particular account differs from that found in the earlier editions. We will let Oviedo 7 This account does not seem to have been preserved. At any rate it is not to be found in MacNutt’s translation. No. 627]. STUDIES IN ECHENEIS OR REMORA 299 speak for himself, and his account is’ all the more inter- esting and valuable because he gives certain details as to the training and care of the fisherman fish which are absent from the other accounts, and of which he seems possibly to have had some personal knowledge. There is a fishing of these Manati and of the tortoise in the islands of Jamaica and Cuba, which, if what I shall now say were not so public and well known, and if I had not heard it from persons of great reli- ability, I should not dare to write. And also it is believed that when there were many Indians, natives, on the island Espagnola, they also caught these animals with the Reversus fish. And since the discussion of the history has brought me to speak of the animal, the Manati, it is better that it is to be known that there are some fish as long or longer than a palma, which they call the Reversus fish, ugly in appearance but of great spirit and intelligence, which sometimes happens to be caught in their nets along with other fish. This is a great fish and among the best in the sea for eating, because it is dry and firm and without watery parts, or at least it has very few; and many times I have eaten of it and so am able to testify of it. When the Indians wish to tame and keep any of these Reversus fishes for their use in fishing, they catch it small and keep it always in salt water from the sea, and there they give it food and make it tame, until it is of the size which I have said or a little more, and fit for their fishing. Then they take it out to sea in the canoe or boat, and keep it there in salt water and fasten to it a cord delicate but strong. Then when there is seen a tortoise or any of the large fish which abound in these seas, or some of these Manati or whatever it may be that happens to go on the surface of the water in such a way as to attract attention, the Indian takes this Reversus fish in his hand and strokes it with the other, and tells it to be manicato, which means strong and of good courage and to be diligent, and other words exhorting it to bravery, and to see to it that it dare to grapple with the largest and best fish that it may find there [where the fishing is to take place]. And when the Indian sees that the best time has arrived, he lets it go and even throws it in the direction of the large fish. Then the Reversus goes like an arrow and fastens itself on the side of a turtle, or on the belly, or wherever it can, and thus clings to it or to some other large fish. This one, when it feels itself seized by the little Reversus, flees through the sea in one direction or another; and in the meantime the Indian fisher- man lengthens the cord to its full length, which is many fathoms, and at the end of this is fastenéd a stick or cork that it may be for a signal or buoy which will remain on top of the water. In a little while the ` Manati or turtle, to which the Reversus has attached itself comes to the 300 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LII shore, and then the Indian fisherman begins to draw the cord into his canoe or boat and when there are but a few fathoms left, he commences to draw it in carefully and slowly, guiding the Reverso and the prisoner to which it is attached until they reach land and the waves of the sea throw them out. The Indians who are engaged in the fishing leap out on land and if the prisoner is a tortoise they turn it over even before it has touched the ground and place it high and dry because they are great swimmers; and if it is a manati they harpoon, wound and kill it. When the fish has been taken to the land it is necessary very carefully and slowly to release the Reverso which the Indians accomplish with soft words, giving it many thanks for what it has effected, and thus they release it from the other large fish which it captured and to which it is so strongly attached that if it were forcibly removed it would be broken to pieces; and thus in the manner I have described are taken these large fish for whose chase and capture it seems that nature has made the Reverso the sheriff and executioner. It has some scales similar to the corrugations [grades] such as are found in the palate or upper jaw of man or horse and therewith certain spines very thin, rough and strong, whereby it attaches itself to the fish it seeks. And the Reverso has these scales or corrugations full of these spines over the greater part of the outer body, especially from the head to the middle of the body along the back and not on the belly, but from the middle of the body up; and from this circumstance they call it the Reverso because with its shoulders it seizes, and fixes itself to fishes. So eredulous is this generation of those Indians that they. believe the Reverso well understands human speech and all those words of encour- agement the Indian says before releasing it for an attack on the tor- toise, manati or other fish, and that it understands also the thanks they afterward give it for what it has done. This ignorance arises from a failure to comprehend that this is a natural characteristic, because it happens many times in the great ocean as I have frequently witnessed, that when a shark or tortoise is captured, Reversos, without having been directed, are found attached to these fish and are broken to pieces on detaching them. From which we may infer that it is not in their power to release themselves after they have attached themselves except after an interval of time or from some other cause I have not determined; be- cause one must think that when the shark or tortoise is taken the Reversos attached thereto would flee if they could. The fact is, as I have said above, for each animal there is its constable. In 1527, Benedetto Bordoni published his ‘‘Isolario.”’ In it is a brief account of the fishing in that locality called Queen’s Gardens. It seems to be an abbreviated tran- seript from Peter Martyr and adds nothing new, save a No. 627] . STUDIES IN ECHENEIS OR REMORA 301 map of Cuba, showing the islands off the southern coast among some of which the fishing, with the Guiacan was observed. This seems to be of enough interest to be re- produced herein as text-figure 3. In 1553, Gomara published at Medina del Campo his ‘‘Historia General de las Indias.” On folio XIIIT is found an abbreviated copy of Oviedo’s account of the Reversus fish, but as it contains nothing new it need not detain us. S Trey oe A TEXT-FIGURÐ 3. The Island of Cuba with the Jardinellas de la Reina to the south. (After Bordoni, 1527.) The greatest of the encyclopedic writers on natural history in the Renaissance times was the Swiss, Konrad Gesner, who was too good a searcher for the marvellous to let such a story as this escape him. His account (1558) is a somewhat abridged but yet almost literal translation of Peter Martyr. However, he gives us a figure of a hunting scene, showing how this fisherman-fish was used, and this is reproduced herein as Fig. 4, Plate I. The Reversus fish is shaped like an eel and has a great bag or pouch attached on the back of its neck. This pouch has just been thrown over the head of what appears to be a seal (probably meant for a manatee), while a turtle looks on in amazement from one side. Inthe background in this PLATH I Fic. 1. Soned disk of Remora. “After Jordan and Evermann, 1906. Fie. 2. Leptech e rda Eve Fis. 3 reai brachyptera. After Jordan and Eve Fic. 4. The first known figure of fishing with the fistierman fish, After Ges- ner, 1558. No. 627] STUDIES IN ECHENEIS OR REMORA 303 boat are the fishermen, one of whom holds one end of a line the other end of which is tied around the anterior part of the body of the eel-like fish. In a sort of post- script Gesner refers to another hunting-fish which is similar to but smaller than the above. This reference, however, is not clear. The first user of the name Guiacan for our fish was Peter Martyr; other and later writers take the name from him. Considerable effort was made to run down this word and to ascertain its meaning. It was finally found in Bachiller y Morales’s ‘‘ Cuba Primitiva’’ ie Here we are told that Guiacan was the name the Indians gave to the fish which the Span- iards called Reverso, and which served them in fishing; because tied by the tail, they fixed themselves to the tortoise and other prey which they did not release, rendering thus a useful service. Earlier than Bachiller y Morales, another writer, Ray- mond Breton (1665), calls the huntsmen fish ‘‘Iliouali’’ and says that it is a fish which has on its head a mem- branous plaque, and if it attaches itself to the canoe it can with difficulty be removed save by breaking it into fragments. That part of Gesner’s ‘‘Natural History of Animals’’ which has to do with fishes was worked over in German and published in 1575 as ‘‘Das Fischbuch.’’ In it on page L is found the figure of the hunting scene just re- ferred to and an abbreviated account of the use of the fish as a living fish-hook. Here also there is an account of 8 Every effort has been made to ascertain the original of this figure. Presumably it is from an insert in some contemporary map or similar pub- lication, Dr, Eastman personally made a search through the rich collection of Americana in the New York Publie Library, the able curator of which, Mr. V. G. Paltsits, had to confess himself at a loss. I myself have worked through the collection of reproductions of old maps in the same library but in vain. Finally the question was submitted to Mr. E. A. Reeves, the learned curator of maps of the Royal Geographical Society, London, who courteously made a lengthy search through all the old maps under his care Finding nothing he passed the question along to the authofities of the British Museum, who in turn could give no co So the origin of this interesting and oldest figure still remains a mystery 304 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII another Reversus. Apparently herein Gesner has mixed certain data from Oviedo with the legends of another Reversus covered with sharp spines. It seems that in the writings of these old Spanish his- torians two fishes are described called Reversus ;? one the anguilliform kind, having a pouch or sucker on its head, evidently a Remora, or, since it grows larger, an Eche- neis; the other the squamous kind covered with scales bearing long spines, evidently the swell fish, Diodon. Concerning these fishes Dr. C. R. Eastman has written several interesting and valuable papers to which the at- tention of the reader is called. (See Bibliography, East- man 1915, 1915a, 1916.) We next hear of the Reversus in the writings of one Antonio Galvano. His book, ‘*The Discoveries of the World from their first Original unto the Yeare of our Lord 1555,’’ was published in the original Portuguese in 1563 under the editorship of his friend, F. Y. Sousa Tavares, and translated and reprinted at London in 1601 by Richard Hakluyt. Neither of these editions being available. Ihave had to content myself with the Hakluyt Society’s reprint'® found in Vol. 30, 1862, as edited by C. R. D. Bethune. Here there is a short paragraph in which the use of the anguilliform eel is attributed to the squamous form. Nothing new is added and no quotation will be given. 9 The reversus or ‘‘upside down’’ fish was undoubtedly so named because when attached to the carapace of a turtle its belly was turned upward or outward, as also when it was attached to the side of a fish—in any case its natural position was reversed. Diodon when it yaa its belly with air floats at the surface belly up, hence it e was a Reve fish. 10 Tt is interesting to note that in the Hakluyt an i se Reverso story is put in square brackets. This considerably confused me and lest others be similarly thrown off the track it seems well to add this note from Mr. C. K. Jones of the Library of Congress, face agi che publishing his 1601 edi- tion was unable to find the original. iety in preparing its 1862 edition secured a copy of the Gena. eee SE. of 1563 from John Carter Brown; and from this copy the Portuguese text was printed.’’ It seems that Hakluyt included in his 1601 edition the Reverso story from original histories, However, in the original Portuguese text, Mr. Jones finds the Reverso story without brackets. No. 627] STUDIES IN ECHENEIS OR REMORA 305 We next hear of the fisherman-fish in Herrera’s ‘‘ His- toria Generale de las Indias Occidentales’’ published in 1601. In Capt. John Stevens’s translation we read: ` They [the Indians] fished on, and took some fishes they called reves,™ the biggest of them about the size of a Pilchard, having a roughness on the belly [?], with which they cling so fast, wheresoever they first take hold, that they must be torn in pieces before they can be torn off again. They ty’d these by the Tail with a small Thread, about two hundred Fathoms more or less in Length, and the Fish swimming away on the Surface of the Water, or but a little under it, when it came to where the Tortoise was in the Water, it clung to the under Shell thereof, and then the Indians drawing the thread, took a Tortoise that would weigh a hundred Weight, or upwards. After the same manner they took Sharks, which are most cruel bloody Fishes that devour Men. Next comes Ramusio, whose ‘‘Della Historia dell’ Indie’’ bears date Venetia, 1606. This appears to be merely a translation into Italian of Oviedo’s Spanish work. At any rate it adds nothing-to our knowledge of the hunting-fish, and may be passed over with this brief notice. Another of the ‘‘fathers’’ of ichthyology is Aldro- vandi, whose great work was published in 1613. He figures and describes both kinds of the Reversus. In general he follows Peter Martyr, but it is very clear that he copies Gesner. However, he has had Gesner’s fishing scene redrawn, as may be seen from the reproduction of it herein (Fig. 5, Plate II). The boat and boatman are omitted, as is the cord around the neck of the fish, the seal-like animal has been replaced by another probably intended to represent a manatee, the turtle is entirely different, and lastly the head of the Reversus is not at all that of Gesner’s figure. This is much larger, the teeth are more marked, the upper jaw has a hooked beak; and the bag of skin comes more distinctly off the top of the head, and is smaller at the base and has more longitu- dinal striations. And yet for all these changes it is plainly Gesner’s figure. 11 Reves is of course a variant of the word Reversus, an abbreviation possibly, daah arig y ? e E ie 73 8 PLATE II Fic. 5. The Indian anguilliform Reversus. After Aldrovandi, 1613. Fic. 6. Reversus or Guiacanus, according to Nieremberg, $ Fic. 7. Fishing with the Reversus, from Ogelby’s “ America,” 1671. No. 627] STUDIES IN ECHENEIS OR REMORA 307 In another place Aldrovandi gives a figure of the spinous Reversus, but in his account of this form he gets his data badly mixed since much of it is the data which Peter Martyr ascribes to the anguilliform variety. In neither account does Aldrovandi offer anything new. We now come to a Spanish work published in Mexico City five years before the Pilgrims landed on Plymouth Rock and when Jamestown was but eight years old. This is Hernandez’s work (1615) on the nature and virtues of the plants and animals used in the practise of medicine in New Spain. How he brings in the Remora is not clear, but he attributes his account to Oviedo, the actions of whose anguilliform Reversus he describes in his (Oviedo’s) own words. However when he attempts to further describe the fish he gets his account tangled up with that of the porcupine fish. He does not seem to ‘have ever seen either fish. In 1635, Joannes Eusebius Nieremberg, a Jesuit priest, who was professor of physiology in the Royal Academy of Madrid, published his ‘‘Historia Naturae’’ in folio form. This is a compilation of not very great value, the less so because the references are not set forth clearly. Our interest in his book, in which he quotes Peter Martyr, Oviedo, Hernandez and another to be referred to later, is chiefly centered in his figure of the Reversus or Guiaca- nus. This is reproduced here as Fig. 6, Plate II. This is plainly Gesner’s figure with the addition of a sort of saw-toothed mane on the anterior dorsal region. Ogilby, whose huge tome was published in 1671, had evidently never seen the Guiacan, but he inserted on page 49 of his ‘‘America’’ such a quaint and interesting figure of his conception (or his artist’s) of how this fishing was carried on, that this is reproduced herein as Fig. 7, Plate IT. The Dutchman, Th. van Brussel, in 1799 published a very interesting account of the Reversus; but a careful translation of his Dutch shows that it is but a translation of Martyr and Oviedo, and further that he confuses the 308 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII anguilliform and squamous forms of the Reversus—a figure of the latter being given. He also need not de- tain us. From this time on a long succession of writers repeat the tale. Thus we find it in Shaw’s ‘‘Zoology,’’ Vol. IV, 1803; Humboldt’s ‘‘Essai Politique sur 1’Ile de Cuba”’ (1826), his ‘‘Receuil d’Observations de Zoologie et Ana- tomie Comparée (1833) and in the ‘‘Personal Narra- tive’’ (English translation, 1860). We also find it in most if not all of the ‘‘Lives’’ of Columbus, notably Irving’s (1828), Winsor’s (1892), and last and best Thacher’s (1903). To these foregoing accounts we may add a brief note which may be of interest. Bernabe Cobo was a Spaniard (born 1582, died 1657) who wrote his ‘‘ Historia del Nuevo Mundo” and at his death left it in manuscript where it remained until found, edited and published by the Span- ish naturalist, Mareos Jimenez de la Espada, towards the close of the last century. Volume II, Sevilla, 1891, contains Cobo’s story which turns out to be the familiar paraphrase of Oviedo’s account. Absolutely nothing new is added. We now come to a consideration of the sources of the various accounts of the use of the sucking fish as a living fish-hook in the West Indies. First of all plainly these later accounts are all echoes of Peter Martyr, or of Oviedo, or of both. Then these further questions nat- urally arise: Is Peter Martyr’s ‘‘Decade of the Ocean”’ in 1511 the first account published? And secondly what is the ultimate source of these earliest accounts? In answering these questions I have had three invaluable sources of information. The one is Justin Winsor’s keenly critical life of Christopher Columbus, the second is John Boyd Thacher’s monumental work on Columbus (Vol. II, 1903) and the third is the continued advice and unfailing help of my friend, the late Dr. Charles R. East- No. 627] STUDIES IN ECHENEIS OR REMORA 309 man.'* Dr. Eastman became interested in the subject while working on the great ‘‘Bibliography of Fishes” published by the American Museum of Natural History, and finding that I was collecting data for a series of papers on Echeneis most courteously turned over to me invaluable material and aided me in every possible way. At the very time when I was slowly tracing these accounts backward towards their ultimate source, Dr. Eastman in the most brilliant fashion ran these stories down to the original recorder himself. First of all let us see if Martyr’s account in 1511 is the first published account of the interesting phenomenon. To this the answer must be ‘‘No!’’ Dr. Eastman sent me the following extract from ‘‘Libretto de Tutta la Navigatione de Re de Spagna et de le Isole et Terreni Novamente Trovati,” Venezia, April, 1504 [‘‘A Little Book in Regard to All the Navigation of the King of Spain to the Islands and Newly Discovered Lands’’]: Continuing [along the coast of Cuba] they found further onward fishermen in certain of their boats of wood exeavated like zopoli, who were fishing. In this manner they had a fish of a form unknown to us, which has the body of an eel, and larger, and upon the head it has a certain very tender skin which appears like a large purse. And this fish [biscia] they loosen the noose, and this fish at once darts like an arrow at the fish or reptile, throwing over them this skin which he has upon his head; which he holds so firmly that they are not able to escape, and he does not leave them if they are not taken from the water, but as soon as he feels the air he leaves his prey and the fishermen quickly seize it. And in the presence of our people they took four large turtles which they gave our people for a very delicate food. After Dr. Eastman had sent me the above translation from the Libretto, I very carefully worked over Volume II of John Boyd Thacher’s monumental life of Columbus 12 The recital may perhaps not be devoid of éither interest or value if the steps are set forth by which Dr, Eastman and myself, working — and at a great distance from each other, traced this interesting story back to its original narrator. But it should be said here that man reached the goal first, and that my efforts were chiefly confined to T his results, and clearing up certain details. 310 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. LIII and from it much of the data following have begn ob- tained. Only one copy of the Libretto is known in the world, and it is preserved in the San Marco Library at Venice. Thacher traced the original manuscript copy of the Libretto to the ownership of a man named Sneyd, living at Neweastle-on-Tyne, but was refused even the sight of it much less a chance to make photographs. However the authorities of the San Marco Library were men of different caliber, and Thacher reproduces in his book the whole Libretto page by page. And I in turn reproduce here as text-figure 4 a part of Thacher’s re- | production of the page giving the Reversus story. It is from chapter XV. a Ceri Trouarono dapo! piu auati al- cuni pelcadori i certe fue barche de uno legno cauo come zopoli ch pe {cauio.In to m5 haucuao tn pefce duna forma a noi incognita ch ha el corpo đ aguiila:& mazor:& fupra ala tefta ha cerca pelle teneriffima che par una borfa grade. Er q{to lo tiguno ligato cé una trezola ala fpo da dela barcha p che el nō po patir uifta de aere:& côe uedeo alchun pe fce grade o bifia fcudelera li laffao la trezola:& qllo fubito corre como una {gets al pelce o ala bifcia:butidoliadoffo qlla pelle ch tien fopra la tefta cô laĝil tié tato forte ch {cipar nô poffono:& non lı Jaffa fi nol tiri for de lag: = <4 < ot a third row. The third digit is almost buried in the flesh of the wing, but can be seen projecting slightly towards its distal end. The claw which is present on the first and second digits is not clearly shown Fic. 2. Outer surface of wing of ostrich, the plumes having been clipped off to show their arrangement in rows. The feathers in the uppermost row, the a ng or ‘Feaniges, vary from 42 to 33 in different birds. The members of ia ities, oki the eoid. row of coverts has often a easter missing towards the free end of the row, though not in the wing egien The other rows of coverts, third, fourth and fifth, may also show reduction. The marginal row of the bastard wing may contain from two to seven feathers. No. 627] GERM. PLASM OF THE OSTRICH 325 tion from the maximum 42 to the present minimum of 33. As experiments have proved that the high number breeds true, and as the other rows of commercial plumes vary in correlation with the remiges, the discovery has a great industrial bearing; for it now becomes possible to pro- vide the farmer with a pure line of 42-plumed ostriches in place of the degenerate 36-plumed birds with which he farms to-day, and the entire feather crop will surpass the present one by about 25 per cent. The first row of upper-coverts varies in correlation with the remiges (Fig. 2) but never shows any indepen- dent reduction, while the second row has often a number missing from its distal end, and is clearly undergoing reduction here in contrast with the elbow end for the under-coverts. Again, it is usually stated by writers that the ostrich is destitute of an under-covering of down feathers and filoplumes, yet in every northern and southern bird examined, down in all stages of degenera- tion occurs around the base of the larger plumes of the wing and tail, and in rare cases spreads over a wider area, leading to the conclusion that at one time the ostrich had an under-covering of small feathers like flying birds generally. The third digit displays certain most unexpected evo- lutionary stages. While in most cases it is altogether em- bedded in the flesh of the wing, and can only be seen and felt through the thin skin, yet occasionally its tip projects quite freely, suggesting its former separation, like the first digit which forms the ala spuria. Moreover, in some birds odd feathers are to be found set along the finger, altogether detached from any other series. These are surely to be understood as survivals of a time when the third finger was clawed, free and provided with its own feathers, a primitive condition which is usually held to be represented only in the oldest known fossil bird, | Archeopterys. : The legs and toes likewise exhibit degenerative phases. The African ostrich is unique among living birds in hav- 326 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LILI ing already lost its first, second and fifth toes, only the third and fourth remaining. The outer, fourth toe is far smaller than the inner third toe, and the condition of its Re wae tip a t AA : I re sy aon xt : ae £ fe pia P t em i wt i T 3? ES 3 pa i R Cy I pa » L aa ion, os 1g g : Se fi — TaY |S Aes eee cA / C A e ina 4] SA Hr Sa wo Mm” A mear EY Sr Ge ~~ 5 y yari TA 1 E A . =~ ie TS { m. JUI wK Hi a YA zi Bee -mÁ =A. j = Ws = wh s and foot of North African ostrich Fic. 3. The sag paige toe is greatly ede ap omparison with the of scales a ntinuous. A second brea joint of fre pa pias larger scales being divided into two or three smaller ones. claw as well as its smaller size lead one to infer that it also is on the road to disappearance (Fig. 3). In northern birds the claw: of the fourth toe is frequently discernible, though altog less, never reaching the ground; No. 627] GERM, PLASM OF THE OSTRICH 327 but on only a few southern birds is it ever seen, and then in a most vestigial state, barely showing beyond the skin. What may doubtless be regarded as the first steps in the degeneration of the big middle toe are also displayed. Down the front of the tarsus extends a series of large, nearly rectangular scales, continuous all the way from a little below the ankle and passing over the upper surface of the toe, though usually somewhat smaller where the toe joins on to the tarsus. In a few ostriches a distinct break occurs at the joint, several large scales being altogether wanting (Fig. 3), and rarely birds are met with in which a second break takes place over the middle joint of the toe. One may hazard the suggestion that the interrup- tion in the seutellation over the two joints has an adaptive significance, allowing the parts to move more freely, but we have also to face the fact that the single break occurs in but a few while the double break is very rare. It is presumably a new feature in course of introduction into - the ostrich race, but not yet established for the members asawhole. It involves however a reduction in the make-up of the toe; it is a minus or retrogressive mutation, and may well be the first hint of impending loss of what will be the only toe when the small fourth has gone. Although definite experimental data on all these reduc- tion phenomena are not yet available everything points to the fact that the variations breed true and are therefore germinal in their nature; they are certainly not ordinary fluctuating somatic variations. Proof is to hand that the 42-plumed cock has factorial representation for its high number of plumes. Another similarly numbered hen is not yet available, but in crosses with various 36-plumed hens the average number of plumes of the progeny is 39.56 which is midway between the parents, while the mode is 40. Considering the heterozygous nature of the ostrich where number of plumes is concerned a fluctuating series of this kind is what would be expected. Only one farmer’ s strain has the nearly complete second and third rows of under-coverts, but they are found in all the progeny from 328 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. LIII the strain; all the members of a flock bred from the same - stock have the second row of upper-coverts complete, while in other flocks all the members have a number of plumes absent from the row. Crossing of birds in which the complete loss of the claw on the small toe has taken place with others in which the claw still appears gives results on strictly factorial lines, as also does the cross- ing of birds with and without a loss of the scales. Ina mixed assemblage of any species where only a small pro- portion display a certain character it may be presumed that the latter will be heterozygous with regard to the particular character, seeing that the chances are much against the mating of two individuals each having the character. The heterozygous nature of the bird can be demonstrated on mating with one in which the character is absent, for if dominant it will appear in half the prog- eny and be absent from the others. This proportion with regard to the presence of the claw and the loss of the toe scales has been found to hold in all the crosses. Out of a total of 36 chicks hatched from breeding pairs where one parent was clawed and not the other the numbers were actually equal, namely, 18 chicks were clawed and 18 un- clawed. Out of 11 chicks reared from a pair where one parent showed no loss of scales on the big toe and the other had a single break, 5 had no break and 6 showed the break. It may be accepted then that all the degenerative phases represent factorial changes which have come about in the germ plasm of the ostrich. Presumably the changes in- volve a loss of factors; they are retrogressive or negative mutations. Structures which would be expected to oceur either fail to appear or are seen very rarely, and may then be regarded as survivals, the factorial losses not having yet taken place in the particular individuals.’ Thus, to take the case of the first row of under-coverts, the principle of alternation demands that a complete row of under-coverts should alternate with the row of remiges, The full row actually occurs in a few individuals, and No. 627] GERM PLASM OF THE OSTRICH 329 suffices to prove that this was the condition in the ances- tral ostrich; more usually eight to ten are missing and also fail to appear in the progeny. It is therefore reason- able to assume that the germ factors originally involved in the production of the eight to ten under-coverts have disappeared from the majority of ostriches though they are retained in a few. The fact that all the intermediate numbers can yet be obtained shows the loss to have been progressive. A similar line of argument can be applied all through. Loss or degeneration is in progress in vari- ous directions and differs in degree in different individ- uals, and the losses are the outward expression of internal changes in the germ plasm. Where a loss of factors is taking place it could hardly be expected that all the individuals of the race would be affected at one and the same time. The process would be more rapid in some than in others, some would incur the loss at one time and some at another, and the results from crossing would need to be reckoned with. Hence we can understand the great diversity of stages represented in the ostrich where large numbers are available for exam- ination. It may be hard to comprehend how in the first instance germinal changes can be brought about, but if once effected, their repetition and continuance can reason- ably be expected. Beginning with one or a few birds it is manifest that as the loss in any direction continues more and more individuals will become affected, until in the end complete loss for the race will be achieved. So far as the investigation of farmer’s troops has proceeded it affords strong evidence for the view that only one orig- inal 42-plumed bird now exists in South Africa, so that under natural conditions the extinction of this high num- _ber of remiges would be imminent. The loss of the claw from the third finger is probably only recent. Some text- books of zoology* assert that a third claw occurs, but it has never been found on the hundreds of southern and northern ostriches coming under my examination, al- though specially looked for. 4 Parker and Haswell, Vol. II, p. 393. 330 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vor. LIII In most instances it would appear as if the loss of all the many factors concerned in the production of a single plume takes place simultaneously, as is the case with most meristie structures; for usually the absences are complete plumes. In some birds, however, two or three incom- pletely formed or vestigial feathers occur between the normal feathers of a row and the absent sockets, as if the loss of the individual plume were taking place piecemeal. This condition can be easily understood if we assume that the constituent factors concerned do not all drop out together, but follow some sort of succession. The factors left at any time would then give rise to the part of the feather for which alone they are responsible, and we should get an imperfect or vestigial feather. In any animal vestiges of a structure will continue to appear so long as any of the factors concerned in the original struc- ture remain. It is submitted that degeneration of any complex structure never takes place in a gradual continu- ous manner, as is usually supposed, but by successive steps determined by the manner in which the factors drop out; the appearance of continuity will however be con- ferred if the steps are small enough. . If a sufficient number of individual ostriches were gathered together it could easily be made to appear as if degeneration in any of the recognized directions were taking place in a slow continuous manner, for all stages between the extremes could be obtained. Proceeding by such a method however would give an erroneous impres- sion of what is actually happening. For although all stages do occur they are in reality disconnected, and each stage has been reached in an individual quite irrespective of the others, and represents a separate and distinct germinal loss; furthermore, in the same individual degen- eration in any one direction proceeds quite independently of the other directions in which the process is taking place. It is not the wing as a whole which is undergoing degen- eration, but the constituent parts of which it is made up, each presumably represented by its own factors and be- No. 627] GERM, PLASM OF THE OSTRICH 331 having with a large measure of independence. The losses are continuous for the race but discontinuous for the indi- vidual; and it is with the individual that heredity is con- cerned and evolution with the race. The degeneration phenomena presented by the ostrich in connection with its wings and legs, as well as with its plumage, would appear to provide us with an example of the application of mutative and Mendelian principles to such evolutionary facts as confront the comparative anat- omist and paleontologist. So far as concerns the indi- vidual bird the retrogressive changes are shown to occur | as separate mutations and to follow definite factorial lines, while as concerns the evolution of the race they pro- ceed in a continuous determinate manner. In all prob- ability they take place wholly irrespective of any adaptive significance or consideration for the welfare of the bird, and are intrinsic in their nature and uninfluenced by ex- ternal conditions. Natural selection has probably played no part in connection with the losses, for the greater changes have already affected the race uniformly and the smaller ones which still vary in degree in different indi- viduals will probably affect the whole in the end. Should the loss of plumage continue to a much further degree and marked degenerative changes be set up in the big middle toe natural selection may then be expected to bring about extinction. The chief point desired to establish at present is that as regards the number of its wing plumes and in certain other features the ostrich affords strong support for the view that its hereditary determiners or factors are chang- ing regularly and frequently; they are not fixed and con- stant as are the factors for the structural details of the plumes; one series is in a state of change, the other is non- changing. The great variety and degree of the degenera- tive stages in the ostrich of to-day admits of no question, and that they are the expression of so many germinal differences may be accepted, seeing that they breed true; that they have been effected simultaneously as we find 332 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vor LIL them is inconceivable, and we are justified in concluding that in the past the germ plasm has changed frequently and presumably over a long period. Moreover, we can hardly admit that the various degenerative phases will remain as they are at present, but that further losses in the same direction will follow, that is, the germ plasm will continue to undergo retrogressive changes of a like character to those already initiated. We may have an appearance of continuous change, but when analyzed it will be found to proceed by means of separate factorial steps. It is conceivable that a continuance of the kind of factorial losses now in progress will result ultimately in the complete disappearance of the wings and legs of the ostrich, allowing that the bird could survive the inter- mediate stages, a postulate which it must be conceded is of no mean order. May we not suppose that the limbless condition of snakes and some lizards, amphibians and fishes has come about by the successive losses of germinal factors in a similar manner to that which is here shown to be taking place in the ostrich? The bearing of the germinal changes iavobved in the degenerative processes upon the thesis of Section IV, may be noted. It is in the highest degree improbable that determinate losses of such a widely embracive nature are taking place in response to any environmental stress acting upon the germ cells; rather they may be regarded as the result of some wholly internal physiological cause. If due to environmental stress one could-reasonably ex- pect that in any individual the losses would be taking place in all directions simultaneously, and would have reached about the same degree in all. But among the various rows of feathers, as well as in other parts, the greatest independence in reduction is met with, as if the factors for each were subject to a separate rather than a common influence. VI “‘ Mixing of germ plasms in fertilization alters hered- itary determiners mutually and hence is, in and of itself, No. 627] GERM, PLASM OF THE OSTRICH 333 a cause of genetic variations . . . a purely external agent, the continued selection of personal somatic qualities, will alter the germ plasm.’’ It seems to be generally allowed that at any period the majority of forms of life are static so far as germinal alterations are concerned, while some are undergoing progressive changes and others retrogressive changes. During the present period the representatives of the widely distributed Ratite are unquestionably undergoing marked changes and have been for ages past. The changes are in a negative or retrogressive direction, and express themselves in somatic degeneration, particularly with regard to the wing and shoulder girdle. The living Apteryx is a well-known instance of wing degeneration, as also the recently extinct moas, in which no hint of a wing has been found and a trace of the glenoid cavity only in one species. From the data already submitted we are able to learn something as to the manner in which the degenerative processes are proceeding in the wing and leg of the ostrich, and presumably the same method holds for the Ratite generally. Factors are evidently in process of dropping out, in regular succession, along definite pre- scribed lines, the degree varying much when the entire race is taken into account. All Mendelian writers seem to concede that the fac- torial changes, plus or minus, are not autonomous on the part of the factors, but are ‘‘a result either of wholly internal physiological causes, or of very extraordinary environmental stresses acting directly upon the germ cells.” Though the results of Morgan and his associates indicate that it may yet be possible to understand the manner in which the factors undergo their changes it will always be competent for us to enquire as to the cause or causes inducing the changes. To be complete our analysis of variability will need to get beyond the factors to the force or forces acting upon them. In the previous sec- tions good reason has been adduced for supposing that the losses going on in the germ plasm of the ostrich are 334 _ THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII due to some wholly intrinsic cause, and seeing that it affects all the members of the race and has been operative for a long period we may conclude that it is transmissible and acts continuously from generation to generation. The many stages represented also give some justification for supposing that whatever the cause of the factorial changes may be it varies in intensity in different mem- bers of the race, being less active in individuals where the loss of plumes is small as compared with others in which the loss is greater. For example, the causative agent bringing about the loss of the plume factors must be less in intensity or less active in 42-plumed ostriches than in 33-plumed birds. We may with good reason expect that the selection for breeding of the high numbered birds will arrest the rate of degeneration of the race in this ` particular feature, while on the other hand the selection of the low-numbered birds will tend to accelerate the rate at which the factorial losses are taking place. Where therefore the germ plasm of a race is in a continuously changing phase, as in the ostrich, we can hope to retard or accelerate the changes by selecting individuals differing in the degree to which they are under the influence of the causative agent. It is submitted that in this sense we can say that ‘‘a purely external agent, the continued selec- tion of personal somatic qualities, will alter the germ plasm.’’ ‘We can not hope that the continued selection of 42- plumed birds will in the end give to the farmer ostriches with a still higher number of remiges, as the factors for the plumes beyond these have in all probability disap- peared from the race, and there is no evidence that the cause of the factorial changes is effective in a plus but only in a minus direction. On the other hand the con- tinued selection of 33-plumed birds may reasonably be ex- : ed to accelerate the loss of the remiges, by leading to a more -rapid loss of the factors. Owing to the present degenerative forces at work in the ostrich we can by selection hope to modify the germ plasm in a minus direc- No. 627] GERM PLASM OF THE OSTRICH 335 tion, though not in a plus direction, beyond the present limits of the race. It will of course be readily appre- ciated that this possibility differs altogether from that due to the ordinary selection which may go on in a race of organisms where the germ plasm is static, but where all grades of pure lines may be extracted between extreme limits. Where the germ plasm for a race is static, as demonstrated by Jennings in his work on Paramecium, we can readily understand that no further change is pos- sible by selection within a pure line, as nothing inducing factorial changes is present. If where germinal changes ~ are taking place it is not permissible to think of the factors as changing autonomously we have to assume that some causative agent is present, and may vary in degree in different members and thereby form a basis for selec- tive action. The same considerations can be applied to the state- ment: ‘‘mixing of germ plasms in fertilization alters hereditary determiners mutually and hence is, in and of itself, a cause of genetic variations.” When, for ex- ample, two germ plasms, in each of which the causative agent producing loss of factors is at its maximum, become mixed in fertilization it is reasonable to expect that the agent will be intensified and the hereditary determiners will be altered mutually, and some of them drop out. The mixing will be, in and of itself, a cause of genetic varia- tion, which will be expressed by a further loss of remiges. Though the idea of a causative agent inducing changes in the germ plasm, and varying in degree and also trans- missible, is altogether hypothetical yet it is stimulating to further experimental effort. Of the hundreds of ostriches examined not one has been found with less than ` 33 remiges, hence this number must be regarded as the present minimum of the race. There is every reason to expect that a pure line having this: number only can be built up. If by breeding these together a further reduc- tion of plumes should take place we should then be fully justified in assuming that the factors concerned with the 336 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LII lost members had dropped out from the germ plasm, especially if later breeding failed to restore them; selec- tion would have induced a definite change in the germ plasm. Also if a pure line with 42 remiges were estab- lished and no further increase oceurred we should be warranted in concluding that the factors for the plumes beyond this number had already disappeared from the race and could not be restored; the causative agent could not act in a plus direction. It is unfortunate for such in- vestigations that the ostrich is such a slow breeder. Ex- periments are however being conducted to determine if further reduction in the 33-plumed birds can be induced, while the building up of a pure 42-plumed race is also in progress, the latter having an important industrial bearing. In many respects the degeneration phenomena in the ostrich appear to be best understood on the conception of autonomous changes and variations in potency of the germ factors. In the case of the dropping out of plumes during the chick stage the reduction in potency has pro- ceeded so far as to result in entire loss of effectiveness only from the chick stage onwards, while complete loss of factors from the germ plasm may be regarded as the final loss of potency. May not a variation of potency of factors be at the root of many of the so-called fluctuating variations? The explanation seeks for the loss of factors among the autonomous changes in the factors themselves, while the idea of a ‘‘causative agent’’ throws the respon- sibility for the changes upon some influence external to the factors. Since the above was written certain results have been obtained which strongly support the idea that it may be possible to induce retrogressive changes in the ostrich. As stated, a loss of scales over the large middle toe has’ already taken place in a small proportion. Out of twenty southern birds of mixed breeding one showed a single break while out of twenty mixed northern birds a single break occurred in three cases and a double break in two. The results given below are derived from the mating of a No. 627] GERM PLASM OF THE OSTRICH 337 northern cock without any break and a southern hen in which the break occurs. Of the four offspring reared three are without the break while it occurs in hen No. 179. From the mating of brother and sister four F, chicks were hatched, two of which have a double break in the scutellation, one shows a single break and one has no break. Thus the proportional loss of scales has greatly increased in the F, generation. SCUTELLATION IN F, Cuicks Comparep Wirth Parents AND GRANDPARENTS No Break Break NN. Ae OSE INO. Dire eto acs te eats X —- SoA bon, No DaD cise a — x F. Crosses. Cross-bred cock, No. 182............. X == Cross-bred hen, No. 179 .........-...-. — x F; Chicks. ONG R as py VEE TE PETE N — xx No: ied oe ger ee OE TE hee — x NO. Bo sve ewes ees ge E oes s8 hs —_ xx E ae Stet SiG CEES. iene. T x — From what has been adduced already we may with good reason admit that an inherent tendency exists in the ostrich towards the loss of certain parts of the fore and hind limbs, and the above result may be regarded as highly suggestive that by inbreeding the inherent tend- ency towards the loss of scales can be accentuated along definite lines. The accumulation of fuller data must be awaited before the suggestion can be regarded as more than tentative. ADAPTATION AND THE PROBLEM OF ‘‘ORGANIC PURPOSEFULNESS.’’ II DR. FRANCIS B. SUMNER Scripps INSTITUTION FOR BIOLOGICAL RESEARCH, LA JOLLA, CALIF. IV. Tue PRINCIPLE or TRIAL AND ERROR In RELATION TO REGULATIVE PHENOMENA!‘ Driesch and some other vitalists draw their most ef- fective ammunition from the phenomena of experimental embryology and regeneration. How is it that a frag- ment of a developing organism—any fragment, within certain limits—can produce the whole? How it is that various perversions of the normal course of development do not prevent the attainment of the normal end? How is it that certain adult organs, e. g., the lens of the eye of a triton, when removed by a highly ‘‘unnatural’’ opera- tion, is nevertheless restored, and restored by a process quite different from that in which it is normally pro- duced in embryonic development? At the outset we must make two admissions: (1) that these processes can not be the result of a mechanism spe- cifically adapted in advance to meet these particular exi- gencies, and (2) that they can not be satisfactorily explained by assuming any preformation of the parts which are restored. The former supposition is to be re- 14 The ‘‘trial and error’’ principle na of late years come into the fore- ground of biological discussion, largely through the writings of Jennings. It was, so far as I know, first clearly proposed PIRR not so named) by Spencer (Principles of Fisddose, Vol. I, pp. 544-545) to account for the origin of adaptive responses to stimuli, and was later developed by Bain. There are important points of agreement between the views of these writers and some of those set forth independently by Roux in his classic essay, ‘*Der Kampf der Theile im Organismus’’ (1881). More recently, Baldwin (Mental Development, 1898, Chapter VII; Development and Evolution, 1902, pp. 108-115) has further elaborated the same fundamental idea as that of Spencer and Bain in his theory of ‘‘ functional selection.’’ Various animal es. (e.g:, Lloyd Morgan and Thorndike) have also laid stress on this principle. 338 No. 627] ADAPTATION 339 jected on account of the unusual and artificial character of the operations, which could never have been provided for by natural selection, nor, so far as we can see, by any other recognized principle of evolution. The latter sup- position is sufficiently disposed of by Driesch’s analysis (section III) and need not be considered here. Driesch admits that a physico-chemical machine ‘‘might very well be the motive force of organogenesis in gen- eral, if only normal, that is to say, if only undisturbed development existed, and if a taking away of parts of our systems led to fragmental development” (II, 139). If, therefore, we can explain these critical cases without invoking any principles beyond those believed to be oper- ative in normal life-history, we have disposed of this line of argument. In an earlier section of this paper I took the ground that an adaptive or ‘‘purposive’’ response by the organ- ism, if not guided by past individual or racial experience, must be the result of experimentation. I avoided inten- tionally at the time any consideration of those cases of regeneration and form regulation in which the emer- gency was totally new, and therefore foreign to the expe- rience of the organism or its ancestors. Here a specially evolved mechanism could hardly be invoked. I sug- gested, however, that the principle of ‘‘trial and error” could be applied to these cases: This suggestion was, of course, not new. Such an extension of this conception had already been made by Jennings,’® though it is rather surprising to note that he has given it little further con- sideration in his recent discussions of vitalism. For, to my mind, an explanation involving this principle, seems the only alternative at present to a vitalistic one, or, better stated, it seems to me the only alternative to an abandonment of the search for a scientific explanation. According to the trial and error principle, as applied to the movements of a lower animal, ‘‘behavior that re- sults in interference with the normal metabolic processes 15 ‘ Behavior of the Lower Organisms’’ (1906), Chapter XXI. 340 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Von. LIII is changed, the movement being reversed, while behavior . that does not result in interference or that favors the metabolic processes is continued.’"® The primary “avoiding reaction,’’ in the presence of an unfavorable stimulus, is, of course, comparable with a simple reflex. Its ordinary effect is to remove the organism from the noxious influence. When progressive movements are re- sumed, they occur at random, so far as their direction is concerned, and they may or may not take the organism into favorable surroundings. If they chance so to do, they are continued indefinitely. If not, the reversal of movement.occurs as before. Thus while, to the uncrit- ical observer, the organism seems to ‘‘seek out’’ the opti- mum environment, it really reaches this through a series of accidents. This is as true of a cat, releasing itself from an experimental trap, as it is of a paramoecium escaping from a harmful to an optimum water tempera- ture. In the case of the cat we may be tolerably sure that the animal experiences a feeling of discomfort until the means of escape is discovered, and we find it conve- nient, if not inevitable, to say that her restless move- ‘ments are the result of this feeling. In the case of the infusorian, we are much less sure of the conscious ele- ment, though its introduction is permissible as an act of philosophic faith. In theory, most scientists are prob- ably psychophysical parallelists, but in practise it seems necessary at times to use the language of interactionism. In discussing the voluntary movements of a higher ani- mal, any other course would seem pedantic. But in dis- cussing the simple behavior of a lower organism, such language is commonly branded as ‘‘anthropomorphic.”’ Nevertheless, I believe that its employment even here is sometimes useful in forcing us to keep in view the essen- tial unity of animal life. No protest is raised by the physiologist when thoroughly protozoomorphic language is applied to a vertebrate. Why then should ‘‘anthropo- morphic’’ terminology so shock us in describing the be- 16 Jennings, op. cit., p. 39. No. 627] ADAPTATION 341 havior of a Paramecium? Each is the extension of an article of philosophic faith far beyond the realm of ex- perience. But this is no essential part of our present argument. Let us consider whether the trial and error principle may not be applicable to other phenomena than the bodily movements of animals. Jennings asks: s it possible that interference with the physiological processes may and the like,—and that one of these activities is selected, as in behavior, through the fact that it relieves the interference that caused the change? . It is evident, then, that the organism has presented to it, by the EENT just sketched, unlimited possibilities for the selection of dif- ferent chemical processes. y is a great mass of the most varied chemicals, and in this mass SS of chemical processes, in every direction, —all those indeed that are possible—are occurring at all times. There is then no diffculty as to the sufficiency of the material presented for selection, if some means may be found for selecting it (op. cit., p. 346). Looking for evidence that such a process of selection does actually occur in physiological regulation, Jennings cites the experiments of Pawlow, in which the latter habituated dogs to various kinds of foods and noted the effects upon the digestive juices. In these experiments the adaptive changes in the activities of the digestive glands, fitting the digestive juices to the food taken, do not occur at once and completely under a given diet, but are brought about gradually. . . . This slow adaptation is, of course, what should be expected if the process occurs -in anything like the manner we have sketched (p. 347). Jennings concedes: It is perhaps more difficult to apply the method of regulation above set forth to processes of growth and regeneration. Yet there is no logical difficulty in its way. The only question would be that of fact, whether the varied growth processes necessarily do, primitively, occur under conditions that interfere with the physiological processes. When a wound is made or an organ removed, is the growth process which fol- lows always of a certain stereotyped character, or are there variations? It is well known, of course, that the latter is the case. . . . Removal of an organ is known to produce great disturbances of most of the processes in the organism and among others in the pro of gro . . . Some of these relieve the disturbance; the variation then ceases and these processes are continued (p. 342 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII A line of argument which has points of similarity to the foregoing has been independently developed by Holmes.‘* He believes: The harmonious functioning of an organism is mainly secured by a system of automatically acting checks which we may conceive to act in manner more or less remotely analogous to the governor fe a steam- engine or the forces which regulate thé motions of the plane these cases deviation from the normal is the cause which iene sets up activities by which the normal is regained. So, too, the self-regulation of organisms may . . . be in a measure understood if we assume that their parts stand in a SS of mutual dependence such that the undue growth or functioning of any part is held in check by the reactions thus brought about by other, and especially the con- tiguous structures. If we suppose that the various cells constituting the body have each a different kind of metabolism, and that the products of each cell are in some way utilized by the neighboring cells, so that each derives an advantage from the particular association in which it occurs, we may understand, in a measure, how this checking may be brought about. And here an analogy is pointed out with the relations which obtain in ‘‘symbiotic’’? communities, such as those composed of animal cells and certain unicellular alge. The conception here developed is in some respects an extension of Roux’s intra-selection hypothesis, though Holmes rejects the notion of a ‘‘struggle of the parts.’’ This conception, which derives strong support from re- cent discoveries respecting ‘‘hormones,’’ gives a certain measure of concreteness to that rather vague expression, “the organism as a whole.’ For, despite the many | known instances of local autonomy, we can not doubt that the organism does in a high degree act as a whole. But this ‘‘ wholeness’’ may not be an irresolvable fact, as has sometimes been assumed. It may be possible to con- ceive it in terms of chemical and structural integration. "° This hypothesis, as applied to form regulation, would 17 Archiv fiir Entwicklungsmechanik, 1904 17a To me, such a viewpoint seems quite teeoneilable with the ‘‘ organis- mal’’ conception of Ritter, though Professor Ritter himself (The Unity of the Organism, Vol. I, p. 183) has gone to considerable pains to show the fallacy of Holmes’s ‘position No. 627] ADAPTATION 343 seem to be closely related to that of Jennings, and in- deed Jennings himself views it in this light. It is diffi- cult to gather, however, to what extent Holmes has in mind the principle of ‘‘trial and error.” His compari- son of regeneration with functional hypertrophy does not seem compatible with this principle. ‘‘Remove one of a pair of organs,’’ he says, ‘‘and its fellow increases in size. Remove a part of one of these organs and the remaining portion grows, forms new tissue, and regen- erates the missing part.’ Furthermore, he believes that these phenomena may be analogous with some of those described under the name of ‘‘chemical equilibrium.’’ The decomposition of compounds in solution proceeds until there is a definite relation established between the amounts of the old compounds and the new. If the chemical equilibrium thus established is disturbed by the removal of one of these compounds more of that compound will be produced; and the more rapidly the compound is removed, the more rapidly it is formed. Such an ‘‘automatic’’ restoration of equilibrium as this might seem to be a radically different thing from trial and error. The process by which it is attained would appear to be direct and unhesitating. Holmes says that the solar system, no less than the organism, is a‘‘self-regulating mechanism.’’ Now, in the former, the balance of its opposing forces is effected ‘‘automati- cally” in the sense that any deviation in the movement of one of the parts would result inevitably in a compen- sating deviation in the others. Is the restoration of an organism to ifs norm of this direct and automatic type? Are such processes as tend to compensate a disturbance in the normal functioning of an organism the direct and exclusive result of the disturbance itself, or does this dis- turbance evoke a variety of responses of which the suit- able response may finally happen to be one? The first of these alternatives may be admitted as probable in the ease of such disturbing factors as have been frequently experienced in the past. But how does it happen that certain cells of the iris of a newt become stimulated to division by the removal of the lens? And why should e 344 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. LIII their metabolism become so affected that they give rise to lens tissue, instead of to iris tissue? Can we believe that the iris cells proceeded unfalteringly to this end as a result of the operation? The discussion after all hinges upon the word ‘‘unfal- teringly,’’ and this term has been applied to processes which are beyond the possibility of direct observation. If we grant that a disturbance of growth equilibrium was what led to the reparative processes, and that equilib- rium was in the end restored, it does not seem difficult to admit that each minutest step in the direction of re- storing this equilibrium was selected from a medley of random reactions. Indeed, Holmes suggests that cells which develop in the direction of the missing part receive those advantages which the symbiotic relation afforded the cells whose place they take. Differentiation in any other direction deprives them of these advantages and subjects them to other unfavorable conditions. Nor need it be assumed that these responses are wholly random. Although it is incredible that each type of possible injury has been provided for in advance by a specific mechanism, it seems more than possible that cer- tain reactions have been acquired which are of service in any emergency—a sort of ‘‘first aid to the injured,” as we might say. After these preliminary steps of a general character—which are, as a matter of fact, the common precursors of regeneration'’—the more special processes may be supposed to proceed in a tentative fashion. All that is meant by ‘‘growth equilibrium,’’ in this dis- cussion, is such a normal state of metabolic balance that the growth of each part is checked through its organic relations with the rest. Attainment of this goal would bring the organism into a condition of ‘‘no stimulation,” like that of the protozoan which has escaped from an un- favorable environment. Since we commonly are able to observe only the final outcome of such a process, and overlook the minute steps 18 These steps are frequently retrogressive ones and include the loss of specialized structures. No. 627] ADAPTATION 345 by which it comes to pass, we are wont to believe that the reparative activities move directly toward the end which we observe to be ultimately attained. Thus Driesch tells us that the process of restitution, perfect the very first time it occurs, . . . is the classical instance against this new sort of contingency. .. . Here we see with our own eyes that the organism can do more than simply perpetuate variations which have occurred at random. What we see with our own eyes, as I have already said, is only a series of visible stages in the process of resti- tution. We do not see the inmost morphogenetic proc- esses, physical and chemical, by which this end is at- tained. Perhaps it may seem that the foregoing explanation merely resorts to the familiar expedient of throwing our difficulties back into an invisible realm where they are safely beyond the reach of scientific investigation. I would say first of all that even this type of explanation, which at least speaks in the language of known facts, is preferable to one which frankly abandons scientific prin- ciples altogether. And secondly, I would point out once more the possibility that this hypothesis is one which may in reality be put to experimental test. For any in- dication of a profiting ‘by ‘‘experience,’’ i. e., of a short- ening of the time required to effect a given regulative response, would harmonize well with the hypothesis that the response was at first effected through tentative steps. Indeed, such evidence, even now, is not wholly lacking. It may be well to remind ourselves at this point that the perfect regeneration of missing parts, or the com- plete reconstruction of a mutilated embryo is after all an exceptional phenomenon. Many animals almost entirely lack the power of regeneration, while most injured eggs either die or give rise to abnormal embryos. These facts harmonize best with the view that regenerative processes are causally produced in the same sense as inorganic phenomena, and that they are not determined, in any di- rect way, by needs or ends to be realized. The forma- 346 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vot LOI tion of misplaced, supernumerary and other useless structures, and the occurrence of anaphylaxis, instead of immunization, certainly do not argue for the existence of a ‘‘primary teleology’’ in nature, though, of course, they do not wholly refute it. On the other hand, the occurrence of these non-adaptive responses to growth stimuli is no more inconsistent with an intra-selection hypothesis, such as that here advo- cated, than is the occurrence of multitudes of non- adaptive structures or colors in nature inconsistent with the theory of natural selection. There must be rigid limitations to the operation of both processes. The task which I have undertaken here is not to explain structures and function in general, but the more modest one of try- ing to explain why certain among these are directed to- ward the conservation of the individual or the species. If various other vital phenomena are found to be non- adaptive, our difficulties ought not to be increased. There are cases, it is true, in which some simple phys- ical factor, such as gravity, or the plane of section, may determine whether the actual missing part is restored or a misplaced organ is the result. It certainly seems arbi- trary to offer fundamentally different explanations in the two cases. Now, I have nowhere made the conten- tion that the processes involved in regeneration are wholly random, in the sense of being unrelated to one another and to the past history of the individual. In normal development the processes are doubtless so con- catenated that growth and differentiation proceed in a direct way with little or no ‘‘lost motion.” And every detached portion of such an organism must receive its share of this established developmental machinery. The tendency to reconstruct the whole, to attain the normal specific form, is therefore opposed by another set of tendencies, urging it to develop as if it were still part of the undivided organism. As is well known, the outcome of this conflict of forces varies, depending upon the spe- cies of animal and the time of operation. We may have No. 627] ADAPTATION 347 either total or fractional development as a result. It does not seem unlikely, therefore, that in every case of regeneration the control of the ‘‘organism as a whole’’ is opposed, more or less successfully, by the specific growth tendencies of the various cells and tissues from which restitution proceeds. These might, in consequence, bring about the ‘‘autonomous’’ production of a wholly mis-. placed part.1® Thus the phenomena of ‘‘heteromorpho- sis’? should seem to offer no insuperable obstacle to the views herein set forth. Applied to the ordinary phenomena of regeneration, say to the restoration of an amputated limb, or even the lens of an eye, this hypothesis of achievement through experimentation would seem to make no impossible de- mands upon our imagination. We need only suppose that the absence of the missing part serves as a stimulus to varied and undirected metabolic activities, that such of these as serve to restore the normal condition tend to be continued and that growth equilibrium (absence of stimulus to growth) is not normally attained until the missing part is restored. The case would seem to be not very different from that of an animal finding its way out of an unfavorable environment. In both instances we may suppose the organism to be in a condition of ‘‘un- rest’’ until the end is achieved. This condition may or may not be conceived in psychical terms. If so con- ceived, the notion would be philosophically legitimate, though scientifically unnecessary.” When, however, we consider Driesch’s crucial case of the development of an entire organism from an em- bryonic fragment, the matter is admittedly far less con- ceivable. For this fragment has retained nearly or quite the same potentialities as the entire egg or embryo, in that its career of multiplication and growth is brought 19 This explanation of heteromorphosis is, I think, quite in harmony with that offered by. Holmes (op. cit., pp. 302-303). 20 Cf, Baldwin’s statement (‘‘Mental Development,’’ p. 177): ‘‘the life- history of organisms involves from "e start the presence of the organic analogue of the hedonic consciousness. 348 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. LILII to a close only through the attainment of the form which is typical for the species in question. Why should this ultimate condition of equilibrium be the same whether we start from an isolated blastomere, an irregular frag- ment of a blastula or anormal egg? Does it not seem as if the only constant feature in this case were the end itself? In considering the behavior of a protozoan, the stimuli may vary and the method of escape may vary, but the organism itself is the same. The ‘‘equi-finality’’ of the result—to use an expression of Driesch’s—may be attributed to this fact that we are dealing with the same physico-chemical system, and one of the self-regulating type. But what of our various embryonic fragments? Are they not obviously different physico-chemical sys- tems? Now, after all, the difference between this case and that of a regenerating limb or lens appears to me to be only one of degree. The distinctions relate (1) to the stage in development at which the injury is inflicted, and (2) to the proportional part of the organism which is left to reconstruct the remainder. 1. As regards the first point, we must suppose that at each stage of ontogeny such a state of physiological bal- ance is normally maintained as is appropriate to that particular stage. That the multiplication and differen- tiation of certain cells is profoundly influenced by the presence or absence of other cells is one of the assured results of experimental embryology. One need only cite the difference between the development undergone by an amphibian blastomere which is totally detached at the two-celled stage, and that of the blastomere whose part- ner has been injured by a needle-prick and left in po- sition. ia Thus we have as much right to assume for the blastula as for the adult animal that any disturbance of metabolic balance will be followed by varied responses, some of which will tend to restore the balance normal to that period. The fact that these responses are known to differ radically, following the same type of operation, No. 627] ADAPTATION 349 and that the result is often a very imperfect reconstruc- tion of the whole, lends support to the view that the cells of the injured embryo ‘‘feel their way’’—so to speak— back into a condition of mutual equilibrium. In some cases this equilibrium appears to be of a simple physical sort, as for instance, that which is brought about by the folding together of the edges of a blastula fragment so as to reconstruct the spherical form. But in most cases the factors are doubtless vastly more complex. Once the reconstruction of the normal embryonic form is attained, the difficulties in understanding the further stages of ontogeny are no greater than we meet with in the case of an uninjured embryo—that is, unless we are encumbered by a preformation theory of development. 2. As regards the second point above raised, there is theoretically no greater difficulty in understanding how one tenth of an organism may restore the remaining nine tenths than in understanding how the nine tenths may restore the one tenth. As a matter of fact, in dealing with certain organisms, the size or shape of the piece, or the region of the body from which it is taken count for little in the outcome. But they do count for something, and that something is significant. It has been found in some cases, for example, that there are lower limits to the side of the pieces which may carry out development or regeneration. And in other cases, the position of the plane of section may determine whether a useful struc- ture is formed or one which is wholly useless. But whether or not the size or shape of the fragment count for anything in the reparation of a given organism, we find that the species from which it is taken counts for everything. There must, therefore, be something that is common to all detached portions of an organism which are capable of reconstructing the same whole. The por- tion in question may be an asexual spore or a fertilized egg, or it may be an isolated blastomere or other arti- ficially detached fragment of either an embryo or adult organism. What is this greatest common divisor? Is it a unit of structure or is it a chemical substance? 350 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LII There would seem to be no third possibility, as long as we keep within the bounds of scientific explanation. But a unit of structure may none the less be itself a chemical individual. Modern speculative physics refers all quali- tative differences in the last resort to differences of struc- ture, even in the case of the elements. And it has been suggested that the various specific protoplasms, which are responsible for the slightly different metabolic prod- ucts of different species, owe their differences to stereo- isomers, i. e., substances which agree quantitatively in their composition, but whose enormously complex mole- cules differ as the result of some slight transposition of atoms or radicals.?! To the majority of present-day geneticists there is doubtless a ready answer to the question: what is this something that is common to all detached portions of an organism which are capable of reconstructing the same whole? It is likely that to most of them a completely satisfactory answer would be: the cell nucleus. Thus Jennings," in discussing specifically certain of the ques- tions raised by Driesch, assures us that ‘‘the recent study of genetics has shown that this [the chromosomal] apparatus is the system on which the peculiarities of development mainly depend. This system is not equi- potential; the fate of its parts is not a function of their position; it has a complex structure with a correspond- ing complexity of action; altering any of its parts alters correspondingly the action of the system; irregular re- moval or disarrangement of the parts destroys the action.” Whether or not this aggregate chromatin matter of the nucleus constitutes the minimum divisible of the organ- ism, as recent students of heredity are disposed to believe, is still quite undecided. For protozoa we are definitely able to state that this is not true. Experiments in regen- 21 Reichert, Science, November 6, 1914. This article contains much inter- esting evidence for the enian distinctness of genera and species, and even of individual organism 21a Philosophical Review, Nor, 1918, p. 586. No. 627] ADAPTATION 351 eration show that there must be smaller bodies within the nucleus, each containing the potentialities of the entire organism. Ritter?!» has recently insisted that the con- cept of heredity must be applied unreservedly to these one-celled organisms, many of which are quite complex in structure and undergo a true ontogeny. Indeed, the experimental studies of Jennings and his students have demonstrated the transmission of individual peculiar- ities, both of structure and function. As for the metazoa, despite the considerable evidence for chromosomal — viduality’’ and for the localization of genetic ‘‘factors,’ it seems to be entirely premature for us to assume the existence of a mosaic of parts, rigidly predetermined and incapable of making good a loss. One should recall what happened to an earlier ‘‘mosaic theory’’ of development. To go to the other extreme, it might be supposed that for each form of organism there was at least one sub- stance, or molecular structure, which was typical for it, and which determined its specific physical and chemical characteristics. The other constituents of the adult body would be modifications of this typical substanee, which had lost certain of its original components or acquired new ones. This specific protoplasm would have some points in common with the ‘‘germ plasm’’ of Weismann. It might be credited with the power of indefinite growth and self-division, so long as these were not checked by counterbalancing forces. When completely checked, a growth equilibrium would be established which would represent the normal form of the species in question. The rather vague and indefinite point of view here sug- gested would avoid, however, the tangle of unverified assumptions that are involved in the hypothesis of a ‘‘yerm-plasm,’’ conceived as an aggregation either of Weismannian ‘determinants’? or twentieth-century ‘“venes.’? The admitted possibility that certain mate- rial particles of the nucleus are functionally related to separately heritable adult characters does not constitute 21b The Unity of the Organism, Chapt. XII, XIII. 352 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII a proof that the entire organism develops through the combined activities of such particles. Moreover, even if such a complete germinal representation of adult char- acters were shown to exist, only a part—and a minor part—of our difficulties would be solved. We should still have to explain how the elementary parts of the body came to arrange themselves in proper spatial order and in proper chronological sequence during development. Blocks do not build themselves into houses. Driesch points out that historically vitalism and epigenesis have always been closely related, while the mechanistic school has commonly adopted some form of preformationism. Such a connection is far from being logically necessary, however. To me it would seem that preformation lent itself most readily to vitalism—to the notion of a builder who put the blocks together. In our particulate theories of organic differentiation, we commonly leave out of ac- count the spatial and chronological relationships of the parts, or rather we take them for granted. We assume that somehow our ‘‘organismules’’ will find their way to their proper places at the proper moments, just as in a laboratory experiment the experimenter himself sees to it that everything is at each moment just where it be- longs. 5 Let us return to an illustrative case, already consid- ered, and ask why no one has ever seriously proposed a . preformation theory of the earth’s origin. Most mod- erns (M. Bergson is an exception) believe that our pres- ent world was the inevitable outcome of forces that were inherent in a fairly homogeneous molten mass, interact- ing with those of its cosmic environment. It has never been thought necessary to invoke the aid of special ‘‘de- terminants’’ to account for the various geographic and geologic features of our planet’s structure. In dealing with inorganic things we are content to let our analysis rest, in the lack of more detailed information, with the acceptance of such general principles as ‘‘creative syn- thesis’’ or the ‘‘multiplication of effects.” We simply No. 627] ADAPTATION 353 have to admit that differentiation means just this fact of de novo formation. Otherwise it means nothing at all. We must, however, recognize certain essential differ- ences between the development of a sea-urchin from an egg and that of our world from the structureless spore which was long ago liberated by its nebular parent. Let us suppose that some experimental cosmogonist, using the refined technique of a Morgan, Roux or Driesch, had skilfully removed about three quarters of our newly formed globe, leaving the remainder to reconstruct itself as best it could. The spherical shape would doubtless have been quickly restored, but is it likely that there would have formed in the ensuing ages just that same arrangement of Europe, Asia, Africa, America and the Islands of the Sea that we now find upon our maps? Un- fortunately it is too late to perform this experiment, but I think that most geologists would expect a much modi- fied world as the result. Indeed, if the excision had been made after the mixture of molten substances had begun to separate we should be perfectly certain that a quite ‘‘abnormal’’ world would have been the outcome. All this may be granted. Let us ask another question. Why is it that no modern thinker?? has set forth a preformation theory of racial evolution? It is only in accounting for individual devel- opment that this has been thought necessary. Yet the same paradox of de novo formation would seem to con- front us in both cases, while other essential points of re- semblance between phylogeny and = have often been pointed out. One difference, doubtless, is that every process of phy- logeny is regarded as a unique thing, while ontogeny is merely the nth reduplication of a known type, the char- acter of which can be stated in advance. Hence it is that we are satisfied to resign the former process to the realm of ‘‘chance,’’ while the latter we come to look on as deter- mined in advance. Another difference seems to be that we look upon racial evolution as largely swayed by exter- 22 We must except Bateson. 354 ) THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou LIII nal factors, of the haphazard sort which operate in the realms of geography and meteorology; while individual development appears to be swayed chiefly by internal factors, and to pursue its preordained course in a high degree independent of the outside world. But where in all this is the necessity for preformation? That two specific types of protoplasm, under identical conditions of environment, will give rise to widely differ- ent organisms implies, of course, considerable difference in the protoplasms. It does not, however, compel us to believe in the existence of correspondingly numerous dif- ferences in the two cases. A single initial difference be- tween two physico-chemical systems may determine a multitude of differences at the end. For example, the presence or absence of a certain amount of annual rain- fall on a given area of the earth’s surface would deter- mine the nature of an indefinite number of other charac- teristics, both geographical and biological. We do not in this case endeavor to pick out a particular element of the cause to account for each particular element in the effect. Driesch’s assumption that any ‘‘mechanical’’ (i. e., non-vitalistic) conception of the developing organ- ism must be based on a preformation of parts may once more be dismissed as untenable. Some preformation there is to be sure. Recent Men- delian studies, particularly the investigations of sex de- termination, make it highly probable that certain adult characters, though perhaps in no case single anatomical structures, are represented by spatially separated parti- cles in the nucleus. Furthermore, a certain amount of ‘‘promorphology’’ has been demonstrated in the cyto- plasm of the unfertilized egg, though this is perhaps to be regarded as representing merely an early stage in individual development. I feel bound to express the be- lief, however, that many recent students of Mendelian inheritance have carried their factorial speculations far beyond the evidence, and that their detailed localization of representative particles may prove in the future to have more interest for psychology than for genetics. We No. 627] ADAPTATION 355 are dealing with a field in which ever more minute differ- ences are being distinguished—many of them by purely subjective tests—and one in which the ratio of inference to observed fact is ever lengthening. May it not be that we have here hitherto unsuspected possibilities of self- -` deception on the part of even our most competent inves- tigators? The subject is one which seems to me to de- serve more attention than it has received. On the whole, we are not compelled to assume the existence of any more preformation than can be experi- mentally demonstrated. And it may be regarded as settled that we have no parcelling out of ‘‘deter- minants’’ to appropriate cells during ontogeny, such as Weismann imagined. The ‘‘sex chromosomes,’’ which seem to be the best authenticated instances of material bearers of hereditary traits, do not pass into definite body cells in the course of development and thus give rise to the primary and secondary organs of sex. Rather are they to be found distributed in every cell of the body. The assumption that they set free their characteristic determinants only in particular cells has no experimental or observational foundation. Now, I am quite aware that any such ‘‘intra-selection”’ hypothesis of organic regulation as has here been advo- cated will be rejected by a large proportion of biologists on the ground that it is entirely superfluous. Various types of self-regulating mechanisms have been found in the non-living world, and the phenomena of growth and regeneration have long been known to be duplicated in crystals. Przibram has gone to considerable lengths in pointing out analogies between the behavior of the so- called ‘‘fluid erystals’’ and that of a regenerating organ- ism.2* And these analogies are reinforced by further ones, based upon the regeneration of crystals of hemo- globin. Many characteristically ‘‘vital’* phenomena were 23 (Archiv fiir Entwicklungsmechanik, October 16, 1906.) Likewise Tor- rey (Scientific Monthly, December, 1915) has discussed some interesting analogies between certain inorganic phenomena and the processes of ‘‘ accli- matization’’ and ‘‘regulation.’’ 356 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII observed by him in these studies, among which the most impressive was doubtless the making over of a softened hemoglobin crystal by a process of ‘‘morphallaxis,’’ i. e., the readjustment of the matter already contained in the fragment. There must thus be recognized in these non- living masses of matter a tendency toward the attain- ment of a specific form. And it seems plain that this tendency may realize itself in more than one way. Yet we should never, in this case, think of proposing any hypothesis of ‘‘trial and error,’’ nor speak of the choice by the crystal of ‘‘means”’ to an ‘‘end.’’ Now, I will hasten to express my own belief that the phenomena in the two eases do not differ in any very fundamental way. I am disposed to regard the regen- eration of a crystal, the reconstruction of a mutilated organism, and the solving of a problem by a mathemati- cian as members of a single series of increasing com- pleaity. They have in common the reattainment of a condition of equilibrium which has been overthrown. The fact that the organism is possessed of life, or that the mathematician has a conscious end in view do not alter the situation. Such a ‘‘regulative’’ tendency in the inorganic world is recognized by physical chemists as the ‘‘principle of mobile equilibrium,’’ or the ‘‘theorem of Le Chatelier.’’ As stated by Lewis,?** this law asserts that ‘‘when a factor determining the equilibrium of the system is altered, the system tends to change in such a way as to oppose and partially annul the alteration in the factor. The same idea is conveyed by saying that every system in equilibrium is conservative, or tends to remain un- changed.” Bancroft?*» has given to this principle the dignity of a ‘‘universal law,” pointing out analogies in the realms of biology, sociology and economics. More recently, its importance in ecology has been urged by Adams.*”° 23a ‘(A System of Physical Chemistry,’’ Vol, II, 1916, pp. 140-141. 23b Science, Feb. 3, 1911. 23° AMERICAN NATURALIST, Oct—Nov., 1918; Jan—Feb., 1919. _ No. 627] ADAPTATION 357 In the regeneration of the more familiar type of erystal, the latter doubtless goes about its task ‘‘un- hesitatingly,’’ we may believe. But this is not true of every inorganic system. ‘‘In a stream [of water],’’ says Jennings, ‘‘opposing actions of all sorts are com- batted in ways almost as varied as in organisms: a hole is filled up, a dam overflowed, an obstacle circum- vented, another obstacle floated away, a bank of earth undermined or cut through; and the stream finally reaches the sea.’’24 Must we not recognize important points of resemblance between such behavior and that of a penned-up cat, scratching wildly at the objects in its cage until finally a way out is found? But if we admit this essential unity between the living and the non-living in respect to their method of correct- ing a disturbed equilibrium, why should we have resort in one case more than the other to a theory of ‘‘contin- gency’’ as regards the relation of means to end? Why may we not suppose the regulative processes of proto- plasm to proceed as directly toward a goal as those of a crystal? Answering the first question, I would say that the con- ception of contingency has been introduced into this dis- cussion merely in the sense of a denial of teleology. Such a denial has been deemed necessary only in the case of organic phenomena. For inorganic events are seldom thought of as governed by ‘‘ends,’’ and the question of ‘‘means’’ does not therefore arise. But in this respect there is really no difference between the living and, the non-living. The reason why the regulative processes of protoplasm probably do not proceed as directly toward a goal as those of a crystal lies, I believe, in the vastly greater complexity of the former. But it does not seem likely that any rigid distinction can be drawn. If it is really true that a damaged crystal of hemoglobin can restore its original form without the taking on of new material, it seems hardly likely that this rearrangement is effected 24 Johns Hopkins University Circular, 1914, No. 10, p. 16. 358 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. LIN by the simple transfer of material from one point to an- other along the straightest possible paths. There is doubtless much random molecular movement which serves only to retard the consummation of the process. The more complex the system with which we are deal- ing, the more of these ‘‘fortuitous’’ steps will intervene between overthrow and recovery of equilibrium. The chances that an entirely new disturbing factor will di- rectly call forth the means to its own removal will corre- spondingly decrease. -The morg plainly, therefore, will the adjustment proceed in an ‘‘experimental’”’ fashion.” Processes which favor the restoration of equilibrium (7. e., Which satisfy the need) will be accelerated; those which work in a contrary direction will be retarded. At this point it may be profitable to cite certain closely related utterances of Jennings :”° The condition which results in . . . regulative action is the presence, in a system, of a constant force, or stream of energy having a uniform tendency or direction (or set of such forces), together with intermittent forces having varied tendencies; whenever this condition exists, regu- lative action appears. . . . When the constant stream of energy is re- strained for some time from producing its usual effects, it overflows in various directions, depending on the distribution of the resistance and amount and intensity of the free energy. It thus produces one effect after another. Often, at the end, one of these effects is of such a nature as to overcome or avoid the restraint; the stream of energy may then continue in the channel thus opened. Has our prolonged discussion now led us, after all, merely to a denial of the scientific validity of the adapta- tion concept? I think not. The concept of adaptation stands. upon the same footing as those of life, organiza- tion, function, food, enemy, offspring, environment, gece heredity and the scores of other indubitable facts with which biology deals. By the use of pedantic circumlocutions, all of these various expressions could doubtless be avoided, and our ideas thus squared with the most rigid demands of ‘‘mechanistic’’ philosophy. 25 Of course, such expressions as ‘‘experiment’’ and ‘‘trial and error’’ must be used in a strictly objective sense, so far as they are given any ex- e. 26 Johns Hopkins University Circular, 1914, No. 10. No. 627] ADAPTATION 359 But would such a renunciation bring us any nearer to the truth? Only if we are ready to regard the whole science of biology as a provisional one, a mere temporary rest- ing place on the way to the more ‘‘exact’’ knowledge which constitutes mathematical physics. How many of us are prepared to make this admission? Before passing on to the next subdivision of our field, a few words are desirable in answer to another general criticism which may be raised against the line of argu- ment here followed. Exception may be taken to the ap- parent assumption that the responses to a new situation, whether physiological or psychological, are wholly. ran- dom. Many responses are so obviously direct and un- varying as to appear ‘‘fatally’’ determined.?* Again, even where ‘‘experimentation’’ or ‘‘trial and error’’is admittedly concerned in the process, the tenta- tive efforts frequently lie within a quite restricted range of possible movements, and from the first approximate the goal to be reached much more nearly than if they were wholly undirected. Thus the experiments of Hob- house** upon various mammals suggest to him ‘‘that re- cent writers have overestimated the effect of pure acci- dent.’’ Furthermore, he concludes that ‘‘the more a success was accidental the less likely were the animals to take advantage of it.” So, too, in learning to throw at a mark, we do not commence by casting our missiles indifferently in every direction, but from the outset we throw them in the general direction of the target. And the same is palpably true when we attempt the solution of a mental problem. The trains of thought are doubt- less ‘‘spontaneous,’’ as pointed out above, but certain more or less relevant trains are favored in advance. It is from these that our selections are made. Now, all these difficulties seem to me more apparent than real. After the first dawn of conscious experience, no situation is wholly new. Every problem which arises contains elements in common with earlier ones which we 27 It is these which Loeb seems to regard as the more typical ones. 28‘*Mind in Evolution,’’ 1915, pp. 236-237. 360 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Von. LUI have already solved. This is the more true the more complex our problem. The ‘‘newness”’ of the latter may relate to a very few features, the residue consisting of elements which, in the last analysis, have been solved in an entirely empirical fashion. And the same may doubt- less be said of those adaptive physiological responses which are generally assumed to be unconscious. As re- gards the fixed reactions known as ‘‘tropisms,’’ I have already pointed out the probability that the predomi- nantly adaptive character of these has been the outcome of racial history and therefore of some form of selection. V. EVOLUTION AND ‘‘ContTINGENCY’’ In the two preceding sections of this paper stress has been laid upon manifestations of the power of self- adaptation in the individual organism. Very little has been said regarding those fixed structural and functional mechanisms by which the more usual needs of life are provided for. The origin of such structures and func- tions—‘‘adaptations,’’ as they are familiarly called— must be accounted for in any adequate theory of evolu- tion. Now, I have already argued that no theory of evo- lution, so far as it is scientific, can admit the possibility that the needs of the organism may call forth in any direct way the initiation of those processes by which these needs come to be satisfied. Let us look somewhat further into this question. The field of organic evolution is one which has lent itself in a high degree to vitalistic and quasi-vitalistic exploitation. From the time of the establishment of the doctrine of descent, there were always persons who, in spirit, still clung to the creation principle, while accept- ing in form the newer ideas. Indeed, among biologists themselves, there have always been those who have seen in organic evolution the working out of a ‘‘perfecting principle,” in a large degree independent of environ- ment. Even Lamarck, who propounded one of the chief naturalistic accounts of this process, admitted that life No. 627] ADAPTATION 361 ‘‘tends by its very nature to a higher organization.’’*® The botanist Naegeli is one of the best known exponents of such ai view. With some, like St. George Mivart, the question has been closely interwoven with special theo- logical beliefs. This writer believed in an ‘‘innate tendency to deviate at certain times and under certain conditions,’’ whic tendency he held to be ‘‘an harmonious one, calculated to simultaneously adjust the various parts of the organism to their new relations.’’ And this guiding hand seems to have been exercised not only in the direction of satis- fying the needs of the organism itself, but in adapting the latter to the needs of man. Speaking of the evolu- tion of the horse, he tells us: The series is an admirable example of successive modification in one special direction along one beneficial line, and the teleologist must here be allowed to consider that one motive of this modification (among probably an indefinite number of motives inconceivable to us) was the relationship in which the horse was to stand to the human inhabitants of this planet.®° Others, like Wallace, have had recourse to such a guid- ing principle only in accounting for the origin of man. In recent years, the philosopher Bergson has adopted a vitalistic theory of evolution, weaving it into a meta- physical system of which an important feature is the essentially creative character of time or ‘‘duration.”’ e see the world of living things moving grandly on through the ages, impelled by a mysterious force, the ‘‘élan vital,” and flowering out spontaneously into a never-ending succession of living wonders. Such a con- ception may stir the imagination, but it does not add to our knowledge. $ Now, curiously enough, this ‘‘teleological’’ factor has been introduced by various writers to explain two exactly opposite classes of cases: (1) the origin of adaptive char- 29 Philosophie Zoologique (Elliot’s translation), p. 239, and elsewhere. Lamarck’s statements are not wholly consistent, however, and I cannot feel quite sure that he had in view any principle distinct from the one with which his name is commonly associated 30 ‘* Genesis of Species,’’ p. 151. 362 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von LIL acters (Paley’s argument), and (2) the origin of highly perfected structures and functions which are not be- lieved to be adaptive in the biological sense, at least to the extent of influencing survival. The musical and ar- tistic faculties of man belong to this second class. Natural selection, as is well known, provides us with at least a formal explanation of the first class of characters, but not of the second. Lamarckism, with a varying de- gree of plausibility, accounts for the origin of characters belonging to either class. That both of these theories are, inlast analysis, theories of selection has been pointed out in section II. But the claim is to-day heard on various sides that _. both natural selection and Lamarckism have broken down completely, and that no other existing evolutionary theories merit serious attention. _So impossible is it for some biologists to square the widespread appearance of adaptation in nature with their own special theories of life that they seek to escape the dilemma by declaring this appearance to be largely illusory. Thus Loeb?! tells us: While it is possible for forms with moderate disharmonies to survive, those with gross disharmonies can not exist and we are not reminded of their possible existence. As a ope: aay the cases of apparent adap- tation prevail in nature. In much the same vein, Davenport*? writes: Strictly, we may say adaptation is not the thing that is brought about, but rather absence of non-adaptedness. Such adjustment as we find is, doubtless, only such a residuum of variants as has not proved incom- patible with conditions of existence. : One might profitably compare such conclusións as the foregoing with the findings of Cannon,** based upon the detailed study of certain adaptive mechanisms in man. To most of us the conviction is doubtless irresistible, not . that such mechanisms now exist because of their harm- lessness, but that they came into existence, step by step, on account of their utility. 31‘*The Organism as a Whole,’’ p. 344, 32 AMERICAN NATURALIST, August, 1916. 33 ‘í Bodily Changes in Pain, Hunger, Fear and Rage,’’ 1916. No. 627] ADAPTATION 363 Taking heart from this skepticism among the biolo- gists themselves, reactionaries are boldly coming for- ward with the assertion that the evolution principle has been discredited. It is certain that the spread of such ideas is not calculated to further the advancement of knowledge. Lack of an adequate hypothesis is not dis- proof of any possible hypothesis. Moreover, it would now seem that some of these ad- missions of inadequacy have been premature. Much of the recent abandonment of the natural selection theory has been due to neo-Mendelian dogmatism. Selection, it is claimed, can only separate strains having different mean characters. It can not change the mean characters of a pure strain. But the experiments of Castle and some other breeders may be cited as evidence that such a contention is far from being established. And even those who reject Castle’s interpretation of these results have been forced to concede that in some cases selection may bring about the indefinite modification of our stock —call the process ‘‘sorting’’ if we will. So, too, the Lamarckian principle occupies the curious position of being dogmatically denied or wholly ignored by a large and influential class of writers, at the same time that others are able to adduce apparently convince- ing arguments for its reality. We certainly have a vast array of indirect or circumstantial evidence for this prin- ciple, derived from an inspection of the actual products of evolution as we find them. And we have a certain amount of direct, experimental evidence which can not be thrown aside as irrelevant or untrustworthy. While, therefore, sweeping conclusions regarding the Lamarck- ian factor are doubtless premature, the dogmatic denial of this factor very nearly amounts to self-stultification. Thus, if we may read the signs of the times, the two chief naturalistic explanations of evolution may survive the fire of destructive criticism and again play an impor- tant part in our interpretation of life. By this, I do not | wish to be understood as arguing that either or both of these theories constitute an adequate explanation (even 364 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST -[Vou. LII in the sense of a description) of how evolution has come to pass. For many years past, I have been endeavoring to weigh the evidence for and against both of these hy- potheses and I have reached the same verdict with re- spect to the two: each is both proved and disproved. It is not that adequate evidence is lacking, as some assume. Rather, in each case, is the evidence well-nigh over- whelming—on both sides. Now, obviously, no single proposition can be both true and untrue at the same time. What is meant here is this. I believe the selection of virtually continuous vari- ations and the inheritance of functional and environ- mental modifications to have both played some part in evolution. And I do not hesitate to say that the evi- dence in favor of such a view is of the same general char- acter as the evidence for the evolution theory itself, and nearly as convincing. On the other hand, it seems no less probable that the operation of each of these factors is strictly limited. In- deed, it would appear likely that much of the adaptive- ness in nature is not adequately accounted for by either process or by both taken together. There may well be other factors the existence of which is as little suspected to-day as was that of natural selection before the time of Darwin and Wallace. But will our explanations remain purely naturalistic, or will they find room for extra-natural directive agents, by whatever name called? Will they, like the two chief | historic theories, base themselves on the contingency of every adaptive variation in structure or function, ante- _cedent to the test of experience, or will they be forced to concede a primary adaptiveness inherent in living matter. Many of those who admit the widespread occurrence of natural selection as a process, are wont to deny to it any explanatory value. To quote a now familiar saying, it is said that the survival of the fittest does not account for the origin of fitness. The real cause of modification, these writers insist, is to be sought in the process by No. 627] ADAPTATION 365 which variations are produced and not in the fact that many of these variations fail to maintain themselves. This argument is so plausible that it seems self- evident. And indeed in a sense it is. But there is an- other sense in which it is quite specious. Truly enough, no individual can survive which is not first born or hatched, or in some way brought into being by its par- ents. And those peculiarities which distinguish one in- dividual from another are largely ushered into life along with it. They exist prior to selection. But fitness is a relation, not an absolute property of the organism. The word denotes merely a certain measure of adjustment to specific conditions of life, and the degree of this adjust- ment we know to vary almost indefinitely. To say that the conditions of life, acting through the selective proc- ess, can not be the cause of an increasing degree of fitness is like denying that a sculptor produces a statue, on the ground that he does not create the stone. It is well to note that even the sculptor’s function is wholly selective. He eliminates certain portions of an unshaped mass of material.*4 The foregoing analogy admittedly fails in one impor- tapt respect. It implies that the possibilities of selec- tion in a given race are wholly unlimited. We know this to be very wide of the truth. The question to be an- swered here is merely whether or not they are completely random in the sense which has been employed through- out this article. Now, some selectionists are wont to deny the com- pletely random character of variation. So far as this is simply a denial of the infinite variability of any species, it is a mere truism. We may perhaps admit the possi- bility that a given strain might, through rigid selection, acquire the ‘‘habit’’ of varying preponderantly in cer- tain definite directions, thus limiting the possibilities of further evolution within that group. And we might even grant that such definitely directed variations might ac- 84T do not recall the previous use of this analogy, but it is such an obvious one that it has doubtless occurred to many. 366 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII cumulate without the influence of selection at all (ortho- genesis). But can we, without departing from natural- istic grounds, conceive of the production in this way of a structure in anticipation of aneed? May we even con- ceive how appropriate variations could be called forth by an already existing need. Of course, much obscurity of thought may be concealed beneath this innocent-looking word ‘‘need.’’ What is a need? It is notorious that what is a luxury to some of us is a necessity to others. Our needs grow with our in- comes. And this line of reasoning is directly applicable to sub-human realms. What an animal has, if this ad- justs it to certain conditions of the environment, may be regarded retrospectively as the fulfilment of a need. Thus eyes fulfil the need of seeing. But can we say that such a need existed before the appearance of visual or- gans? There are beyond doubt still many forms of wave-motion or molecular vibration for which we have no organs of perception. Thus, in a large measure the organism creates its own needs, even in an unchanging environment. The word ‘‘need,’’ like the word ‘‘end,”’ is one which has a distinctly teleological implication. The more factors of the environmental complex an organism is brought into relation with, the better is it adjusted to its life conditions, and—other things equal—the higher position it holds in the seale of life. But these adjust- ments are only thought of as satisfying needs when we come to look back on what has actually happened.*® There is a more limited sense, however, in which the use of this expression involves us: in no such obscurities. All those fundamental requirements, such as food, oxy- gen, protection from enemies, ete., may be termed needs, without there resulting any confusion of thought. Now, anything which. led to the removal of one or more of. these fundamental requirements—say the drying up of a lake—might bring about the extermination of an entire species, unless some adaptive response were made. 35 They may all, however, be ‘properly termed adaptations, as has already been said No. 627] ADAPTATION 367 Here, likewise, we may legitimately speak of the need for some sort of readjustment. Let us, then, restrict the word to anything without which a species would become extinct. With this limitation of meaning understood, let us re- turn to certain questions which I have left unanswered. Can we, on naturalistic grounds, conceive how an appro- priate trend of variation could anticipate a given need; or can we even conceive how it could be called forth by an existing need? The former possibility certainly can not be admitted without frankly taking refuge in principles which lie beyond the range of scientific analysis. The latter possibility has, however, been vaguely implied by some writers on evolution. So far as the ‘‘need’’ might be the result of some marked change in the environment or in the functional activities of the organism, it is credible that new varia- tions might be offered to selection as a consequence of disturbances in the germinal material. But how could these occur preponderatingly in the direction of meeting the particular need in question? Only in one way, so far as I can see, and that way is by the previous adaptive modification of the parent body. For the latter may adapt itself experimentally, according to principles al- ready discussed. The germ-cells could not adapt them- selves experimentally, since the need is commonly one which does not as such affect them at all. Thus, the imperative demand for directed germinal variations—or at least ones of a useful sort—can be met, so far as now appears, only by assuming the transmission to the germ- cell of adaptive responses of the parent body. The Lamarckian principle has the added advantage of being able to account for many of the ‘‘luxuries’’ of organization—adaptations, in the sense of fitting their possessors for a fuller and more varied life, but not of any conceivable survival value. Our own race, as has often been pointed out, is endowed with multitudes of such faculties. But we are sadly in need of direct ex- perimental evidence along these lines. 368 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LIII Biologists of the future may recognize the importance - of determining experimentally whether the germinal variations of a species ever respond to changed life con- ditions in such a way as to shift the mode of any char- acter in the direction of greater adaptation. If sucha general tendency as this were revealed, and if, at the same time, the transmission of somatic modifications were rigidly excluded, we should be brought to a crisis in the history of our science. The question at issue would not be merely the adequacy or this or that hypoth- esis. It would be the adequacy of our recognized scientific methods to deal with such problems. Despite the lengthy arguments with which I have sought to defend a purely naturalistic position, I should not, in advance, be su- premely confident as to the outcome of such experiments. It might, after all, turn out that there was just such an ‘immanent teleology’’ in living things as the vitalists claim. If this should prove to be true, science would have to re-survey its territory and set itself new bound- aries well within the old ones. Such an undertaking, like that of settling once for all the ‘‘acquired characters’’ question, would doubtless be beset by great technical difficulties. But these difficul- ties should not be insuperable. So long, however, as ‘“genetics’’ is held to be nearly or quite synonymous with Mendelism, evolution along dynamic lines is likely to lan- guish. We must grant the enormous strides which have been made in our knowledge of the inheritance of certain types of variations, but the much more fundamental ques- tion of the causes of these variations is almost as far from solution as in the days of Darwin. In conclusion, I would say a few further words in re- gard to my use of the expressions ‘‘contingency’’ and ‘‘chance’’ throughout these pages. It is needless to say that I have not used these words as synonymous with un- caused. I have spoken of an event as contingent, merely in the sense of its being causally unrelated to something else: for example, a variation in relation to a need to be fulfilled. Whether or not, in the last analysis, all things No. 627] ADAPTATION 369 are causally related in an Absolute, or whether the Uni- verse is pluralistic in its nature, need not concern us here. That there may be some measure of pre-estab- lished harmony among its various parts is possible. It has recently been ably argued—and by a chemist, not a theologian—that there exists such a pre-established har- mony between the organic and the inorganic worlds as a whole.* But even granting such very problematic relationships as this, we can not deny that much happens in a purely ‘accidental’? way. No degree of fitness on the part of the environment for life in general can avail to prevent the wholesale destruction of organisms which ‘‘happen’’ into unfavorable surroundings. That all of the special adjustments between organism and environment arose primarily through contingency or chance in the sense here indicated is the main thesis which I have defended in these pages. There may be little of an original na- ture, either in the views proposed or the arguments used . in support of them. But I believe that this essay may serve a useful purpose in bringing together a number of apparently distinct problems.under a common viewpoint. 86 L. J. Henderson: ‘‘The Fitness of the Environment’’ (1913), ‘‘ The Order of Nature’’ (1917). SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION PIEBALD RATS AND SELECTION, A CORRECTION In a recent important publication Dr. Sturtevant makes ‘‘an analysis of the effects of selection’’ in which he ably maintains the current view that the single gene is not changed by processes of systematic selection. His argument rests on a careful experi- - mental study of the behavior of the character ‘‘dichaet’’ in Dro- sophila, followed by a general discussion of other work, my own in particular. I am represented as completely opposed to his view, and so I have been at times, but such is not the case at present. Iagree so fully with his general conclusion that I want to obviate needless discussion based on the misapprehension. I thought two years ago that I had evidence that a single gene had changed in the course of a selection experiment, this gene being concerned in producing the hooded pattern of rats. Inow find this view rendered untenable by further experiments, the results of which are in course of publication. These results show that the supposed changes in a single gene are more prob- ably due to changed residual heredity, which very likely may consist wholly of other ‘‘modifying’’ genes. he crucial experiment was one suggested by Dr. Sewall Wright. The divergent hooded races, ‘‘plus’’ and ‘‘minus,’’ re- sulting from selection, were to be crossed repeatedly with a third race, the hooded character being recovered as a recessive in F, following each cross and its variability compared with that of the uncrossed race. It was believed that if multiple modifying genes were involved, repeated crossing with a pure third race would tend to remove these, in which case the extracted hooded character being deprived of its plus modifiers would be sub- stantially identical with the hooded character deprived of its _minus modifiers, as seen respectively in hooded recessives derived from the plus and from the minus crosses. Well, they are sub- stantially identical, but it has taken some time and a good deal of trouble to establish the fact. First we had to secure a satis- factory third race to use in the crosses, one free from contami- nation of any sort by crosses. This we sought in a wild race. But ordinary wild rats will not breed under laboratory condi- 370 No. 627] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION Stl tions. So we resorted to trapping immature wild rats from a single locality and using these as a foundation stock. Crosses with the plus race were then started successfully, but the corre- sponding experiment with the minus race was hard to get going and so has lagged behind the plus crosses. A report on the re- sult of the plus crosses was made in 1916 (Castle and Wright). The crosses with the minus race were not then sufficiently ad- vanced to show what their outcome would be and this was still true when reply was made to the criticism of MacDowell, as it had been previously when reply was made to Muller. and to Pearl, and subsequently when I addressed the Washington Acad- emy of Science on the rôle of selection in evolution (1917). But since then the minus crosses have given what seems to be con- clusive evidence that the single gene had not been altered by selection, although the inherited complex responsible for the ooded character had steadily been altered in opposite directions and these alterations were permanent in the sense that they rep- resented racial modes, stable so long as the race was not out- crossed. I still have on hand a few representatives of the plus and of the minus races which because of their low fecundity it has been impossible to select further for several generations. The two races are very different in appearance. The plus race shows no white except on the under side and sometimes along the flank. The minus race shows no black except a short hood lying anterior to the shoulders, and in an occasional individual a small black spot or two in the middle of the back or on the tail. Yet the variability of each race is still considerable; as measured by our ‘“‘grades’’ it has not appreciably diminished in recent genera- tions. The somatic differences entailed by the selection experi- ments with the hooded character of rats are seemingly greater than those secured by Sturtevant or by MacDowell in regard to bristle number in Drosophila, yet I doubt not they may be ex- plained on similar grounds. Crossing with a wild race affects very differently the plus and the minus selected races. See Tables I and II. The plus race was much less affected than the minus race. Its mean grade was lowered, by three successive crosses with the wild race, not over three quarters of a grade. The standard deviation was about doubled by the first cross. That is the variability of the hooded character, when extracted in F, from the first wild cross, was 372 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vor. LIL about twice as great as the variability of the hooded character in the uncrossed plus selected race. In the second and third crosses the variability declined somewhat, but was still consid- erably greater than that of the uncrossed race. It was indeed very similar to that of the plus race in the first seven generations of the plus selection experiment. (See Castle and Wright, p. 186.) TABLE I RESULTS OF REPEATEDLY CROSSING THE PLUS SELECTED RACE WITH A WILD RACE Number of Mean Standard Grade Deviation ee Pana verges plus sy R eine + 3.73 36 776 Once extracted hooded Fz young............... + 3.17 a3 Twice Aa] hooded Fy. erp AL UNNA E + 3.34 50 256 Thrice extracted hooded Fz young.............. + 3.04 64 19 TABLE II RESULTS OF REPEATEDLY CROSSING THE MINUS SELECTED RACE WITH A WILD RACE P N Mean Standard Hooded Grade Deviation Young Control, uncrossed minus sea NEEE 16. — 2.63 Sri 1,980 Once extracted hooded Fz young............... — ,38 1,25 121 Twice extracted h F: eg PEE a DANE AGP AE + 1.01 .92 49 Thrice extracted hooded Fz young.............. + 2.55 66 104 The crosses of the minus race were started six generations later in the course of the selection experiments, with animals of generation 16, minus selection series. They show effects much more striking than those of the plus crosses. See Table II. The minus selected race had now attained a mean of —2.63. A single cross, with the same wild race used in the crosses of the plus series, lower the grade to —.38, extinguishing all the changes in mean grade made by sixteen generations of selection, and leaving the extracted hooded character in a highly variable state (standard deviation 1.25, nearly five times what it had been before). A second cross with the same wild race converted the extracted hooded individuals for the most part into a plus group, mean -+ 1.01, but with variability somewhat decreased, standard deviation .92. A third cross with the wild race has given ex- No. 627] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 873 tracted hooded individuals exclusively plus in character, range from + 1.00 to + 3.50, mean + 2.55. The variability has si- multaneously fallen to .66, which is only about one third greater than that of the minus race in the first five generations of the selection experiment. (See Castle and Wright.) One family- containing fourteen thrice extracted hooded individuals has a mean grade for the hooded individuals of + 3.05, which is prac- tically identical with the grade of the thrice extracted hooded individuals resulting from the plus crosses (Table I). It thus appears that three or at most four crosses with a wild race suffice to obliterate all the racial differences which had been induced by ten generations of selection in the case of the plus race and sixteen generations in the case of the minus race. The plus race was changed almost immediately by a single cross, but the change was small (a fact which misled me until the results of the minus were secured). The changes with the minus race were so great that they could not be fully secured by less than three or possibly four successive crosses (eight generations of offspring). The wild race, which we used in our crosses, evi- dently had a residual heredity much more like that of our plus- selected than like that of our minus-selected race. When the hooded gene from either race was introduced by repeated crosses into this residual heredity, the result was to produce hooded races of very similar grade, a little lower in grade than the plus selected race, but very much higher in grade than the minus selected race. It thus becomes clear that the changes which had occurred in the hooded character as a result of selection were detachable changes and are probably in nature independently inherited modifying factors. This is a view which Phillips and I gave as one of two possible interpretations of the results which we pub- lished in 1914. Morgan, Muller, MacDowell and others have in- sisted that this was the only reasonable interpretation which could be given, but I have not been satisfied with this conclusion in advance of a really crucial experiment, such as I believe has now been performed. Meanwhile the probability that the theory of multiple modifying factors is correct as a general explanation of similar cases has been greatly strengthened by the work of Muller, Bridges, Sturtevant and others, showing that genetic fac- tors, having a definite demonstrable position in linkage systems, influence in a particular way the somatie manifestation of char- 374 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou, LIN acters varying „quantitatively or qualitatively. I accept their ; interpretations as correct in the light of our present knowledge. I should feel like apologizing for my own obtuseness in not reaching a similar conclusion sooner, did I not recall with satis- faction how much clearer the réle of selection now stands re- vealed than it did when these experiments were begun, and to the clearing up of the situation I shall at least hope that this rat work has contributed something, if only by provoking inves- tigation. The ‘‘Mutation Theory’’ of DeVries gave us a picture of se- lection as an agency temporarily effective in producing racial changes, but with those changes gradually vanishing as soon as the selection ceased. Johannsen denied within ‘‘pure lines”? even temporary effectiveness of selection. A strictly logical use of Johannsen’s conclusions would have limited their application to such organisms as he studied, self-fertilizing ones completely homozygous for all genetic factors and subject apparently to no new changes in such factors. But the doctrine was straight- way extended in the views of most geneticists to selection of every sort and he was treated as a traitor to Mendelism who saw any utility in selection or advocated its use as a means of im- proving the inherited characters of animals or plants. The situation is wholly different to-day. Through the inves- tigations of Jennings and his pupils on protozoa, of Stout on Coleus, and of Shammel on citrous fruits, the fact is clear that even within clones genetic changes may and do frequently occur and that systematic selection will serve to isolate these and thus lead to racial improvement. Those who have tried systematic selection in the case of cross fertilizing organisms have in some cases noted the occurrence of ‘‘mutations’’ with such frequency as to make progressive change under selection easily obtainable. Emerson and Hayes, in the case of certain pericarp color pat- terns of maize, find ‘‘mutations’’ so common that a wide range of variability results and selection is able to isolate, from such material, types ‘‘relatively stable,’’ but very diverse in appear- ance. Modifying factors are not involved in Emerson’s expla- nation of his results, but rather such instability of a single gene as leads to frequent mutation. Selection experiments with the variegated coat-patterns of mammals seem to involve less abrupt but otherwise similar changes, but modifying factors rather than repeated mutation seems to be the explanation required in view of the results of crosses reported in this paper. No. 627] SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION 375 That selection by one means or another is an effective agency in producing racial changes is not questioned to-day, as it was ten years ago.t The only question now at issue is whether the single gene ischangeable. I am inclined to think, with Sturtevant, that while single genes do occasionally change producing multiple al- lelomorphs, a much more common occurrence is change in visible characters through modifying factors. Whether the direction of genetic variation is controllable, other than by the manipula- tion of modifying genes or the discovery of multiple allelo- morphs remains to be determined. The evidence at present is largely negative. It is undeniable that liability to genetic varia- tion is much greater in some organisms than others, much greater as regards some kinds of character than as regards others, but whether we can produce variability of a genetic character is quite a different question. We certainly at present have to follow nature’s lead rather than to lead nature, as regards the course of evolutionary change. W. E. CASTLE BUSSEY INSTITUTION, HARVARD UNIVERSITY. 1 Sturtevant’s presentation of my views is a bit unfair in that it seems to imply that whenever I have spoken of ‘‘variation in a unit character,’’ I have consistently meant variation in a single gene, whereas in discussing the ease of the English rabbit, I have expressly reserved judgment on this point. In a large part of my experimental work, the question under investigation has been—do the visible characters which conform with Mendel’s law in transmission suffer modification in crosses or as a result of selection? The present opie of geneticists has nd geo! forgotten that this was ever a debatable question. We all admit now that contamination occurs in crosses va that modification may be mee by selection, an only to explain how the contamination is brought about (as by modifying factors) or how the modification is produced in the course of systematic of ieee i i the days when the doctrine of gametic purity was under discussion, such ‘*contamination’’ or ‘‘modification’’ was not admitted. When Sturtevant denies the occurrence of ‘‘contamination,’’ he uses the term in a very restricted sense, not as I have used it in a! Payne sen- tence, nor as it was formerly used in BOPTI discuss: What he means is not change in the visible charac as the iodid i cinchsion of ae but change in a single gene which is Bnet absolutely to limit the anifestation of the hooded character in any form. I agree with his view rer there is no conclusive evidence that this single gene had changed in the course of selection experiments, except in the case of our ‘‘mutant’’ 376 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LII BIBLIOGRAPHY Castle, W. 1916, ae Selection Cause Genetic Change? AMER. Nart., 50. ea Piebald Rats and Multiple Factors. AMER. NAT 1917a. let a le of Selection in Evolution. Jour. Wash. Keak Sci., » P- Castle, W. co. aa “Hadle ey, 1915. The English hie imi the Question : Mendelian Unit- racter Const yee c. Nat, Ac. Boi., Keer wie and Phi ities T 1914. i Spal Rats and Sictis Carnegie Inst. Wash., Publ. No. 195. Castle, W. E., ‘and Wright, Sewall. 1916. Studios of Inheritance in Guinea-pigs and Rats. Carnegie t. Wash., Publ. No. 241. Emerson, R ER 1917. Genetical Studies of Vübpi Pericarp in Maize. Genetics, 2. Hayes, 1917. Inheritance of a Mosaic Pericarp Pattern Color of Maize. Dee netics, 2 - Jennings, H. S. 1916. uot Variation, and the Results of me ey in Uniparental Reproduction in Difflugia corona. Genetics, 1 MacDowell, E. C. 1916. Piebald Rats and Multiple Factors. AMER. Nar., 50. 1917. Bristle Inheritance in T Jour, Exp. Zool, 23. Stout, A. B 1915, The Establishment of Varicties in Coleus by the pug: of Somatic Variations. Carnegie Inst. Wash., Publ. No. Sturtevant, A. H. 1918. An Sgr ow of the Effects of Selection. Carnegie Inst. Wash., Publ. No. 264. THE AMERICAN NATURALIST Vor. LILI. September—October, 1919 No. 628 EGG-WEIGHT AS A CRITERION OF NUMERICAL PRODUCTION IN THE DOMESTIC FOWL! DR. PHILIP HADLEY I Wuen one surveys the field of literature dedicated to the subject of egg-production in the domestic fowl he may well be astonished at the vast number of ways and means by which a poultryman can detect the best layers of his flock. Indeed one exaggerates only slightly to say that there is scarcely an incident in the hen’s daily program, nor an event in her life, that has not been interpreted by some unusually keen observer as a sign of producing ability—good or poor. Was the hen seen to rise early and dispatch a one-hundred-calory portion of mash, together with nine bugs and three worms, before her sisters were off the roost? Then put her down unqualifiedly as an industrious hen and enthusiastic layer—a credit to any poultry house. Was the hen observed to work after hours gleaning the last fragment of grain from the litter, or perchance chasing lightning bugs through the twilight grass, when other union-members of the flock had ceased work for the day and retired to roost? Then register her as one that has her master’s interests at heart, and one that should be vigorously encouraged to reproduce her like. Did the hen lose the yellow glamour of her shanks and beak (doubtless the equivalent of good complexion in ahen)? Did she molt in July or August? Was her comb 1 Contribution 250 from the Agricultural Experiment Station of the Rhode Island State College. 377 378 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Vou. LIII a ruddy red in September? Was her pelvis broad and flexible in April? Did she start laying in October? Did she lay thirty or more eggs before the first day of March? Did she lay 200 eggs in her pullet year, or 500 eggs in three years? Did she lay small eggs or large eggs? By all these signs one may (it is alleged) detect the hen that is (or has been) the good producer. But the curious part of the matter is that, notwithstanding these many signs and evidences of producing ability, the hens of the aver- age poultryman continue to deliver the same number of eggs per year—estimated at about 120. Among this variety of criteria, however, it must in fairness be said that some of the tests are of practical significance. It can scarcely be doubted that, as a rule, hens that lay the largest number of eggs during the ‘winter period’’ (November 1 to March 1), as first stated by Pearl, are the best layers for the entire year. On the other hand, it has been shown by Goodale that the produc- tion during the winter period may be strongly influenced by the time of hatching: the early-hatched hens make the highest winter records—at least they lay the greater number of eggs between the beginning of the laying period (sometimes as early as August) and March 1.? If a hen is entitled to be called a good producer only on condition that she makes a creditable record for two or more years successively—then there is point to the recent contention of other investigators that hens that make a low first year’s record usually ‘‘make up’’ during the second year, so that a three-year production record ap- pears to them as representing the fairest measure of pro- ducing ability. This is of course the equivalent of saying that the number of eggs that a hen lays is a good criterion of her egg-producing ability—a circumstance which no one can deny. But it frequently happens that, for prac- tical purposes, one desires such a criterion as will indi- 2It may be a question, however, whether the ‘‘ winter period’? of Good- ale’s early-hatched pullets may not in reality represent a combining of two laying cycles. His data on production seem to make possible this interpre- tation No. 628] EGG-WEIGHT 379 cate a hen’s producing ability before she has attained that stage in life when economic production ceases; and when, even as a breeder, her further producing days are few. II To the casual reader it will no doubt appear preposter- ous that a biologist should attempt to measure the numer- ical egg-production of a hen by weighing her eggs, rather than by counting them. But the author freely admits that this ridiculous thing has actually been done in his laboratories; and, what is more, that the method appears to work: a flock of hens can be divided into groups, each characterized by a different mean producing ability, as a result of weighing a certain number of eggs at a certain time in the laying year, and subsequently by making cer- tain computations therefrom. The results depend upon the relation existing between egg-weight and egg-produc- tion at different periods of the laying year. These points may be considered separately. deg tt: San Yeo Mar Ayr Mau June July Aua Sept Get Nev Dee Jan “Fete Mar Apr May Tune ‘ a Ton beheld Man. ‘ Ef $ ww ma z E 4 “x 3 Seo a3 ? gz 2 be $ an 3 + $s s Joo £ "i E [$9 H ‘ | E Reo A E EE om ARN z ni | 3 2 | RE SERAN 5 TERA et aN 2 Tees es š we oe - È ow {pa S S Bat---4 b £ =e Sate, = E = ba P F 4 ži k fn 4 L4 > R- A S A AEE E r---4 cx s ys P St ił Se - Savaa Seana ia peg “Riese Saying Xear{(taniere) * (parria) Fig. 1. When the first yearly production of a flock of hens of equal age and condition is plotted on monthly ordinates one frequently obtains a curve such as indicated by the full line in Fig. 1. It is observed that the production 380 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Vou; LIH curve for the year ending October 31 is of the bimodal type.® One of these modes, appearing on the spring of the year (in April), may be termed the vernal production maximum. The second mode, falling in September, may be called the autumnal production maximum. Of these two maxima for the first laying year the vernal maximum manifests the higher peak, in keeping with the heavy April production which is the highest of the year. Be- tween any two successive maxima lies a fundus whose minimum is attained either in July or August, or in No- vember of each year. The late summer depression may be termed the estival production minimum, and the No- vember depression, the autumnal production minimum. In the month of December of the second laying year it will be noted that the curve rises slightly. This repre- sents the increased production of the ‘‘winter cycle’’ or, as it may be called, the hibernal production maximum. Between this and the vernal production maximum of the second laying year is another depression in the curve, following the period of winter production and indicating the hibernal production minimum. This is followed in turn by the vernal maximum of the second year. The presence of these two maxima in the curve of pro- duction for the first laying year indicates that at least twice during this year (closing October 31), once in the spring and once in the autumn, the egg production of a hen rises from its lower limits and expresses itself by laying a larger number of eggs than at any other season. These periods of increased production represent the spring and the autumn cycles respectively. There exists also the winter cycle which is usually manifested with clearness only in those flocks which show a fairly high - mean production. It is a significant productive period, but will not receive further consideration at this time. It may be added, however, that in birds that are fairly high producers and which are hatched sufficiently early 8 These hens were poor winter producers and the expected mode for De- cember-January of the pullet year does not appear. No. 628] EGG-WEIGHT 381 in the year, the winter production maximum may make its appearance as the first cycle of production of the first laying year. We have considered some of the more obvious varia- tions in the curve of numerical production and come now to the curve of variations in egg-weight. Such variations may be considered with reference to the clutch, the litter, the cycle or the year.* For present convenience, however, we shall consider the variation by months—purely arbitrary divisions in the life of the hen, which cut in on, and interrupt clutches, litters and gelea: in such a way as frequently to obscure many of the problems involved. For our present purpose, however, division by months offers a rough and ready division of the year into short periods of time in which the productions may be compared. When all the eggs laid by a flock of hens are weighed and recorded, and the monthly means computed and plotted on monthly ordinates, such a curve of mean monthly variation in egg-weight is obtained as that shown by the broken line in Fig. 1. Such a curve shows that all the eggs that a hen lays are by no means of equal weight. The first eggs laid are relatively small, but the weight increases gradually until a maximum weight for the first year is attained in the month of April. This is termed the vernal weight maximum and may represent mean increase of five grams over the mean weight of eggs for the first laying-month of the year. This maximum forms the first mode of the frequency curve of variation in egg weight as shown in the figure. -After April, the curve of variation in egg-weight drops for May, again for June, and reaches the lowest point in July, at which time the mean weight of the eggs of the flock may be scarcely greater than for the first month of production. Having struck this low point, however, the 4A clutch may be regarded as the group of eggs laid on successive days without an interruption. A litter is the group of eggs laid immediately preceding the onset of a broody period. A cycle is the larger group of eggs laid during any one of the seasonal periods of increased production. 382 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Vou. LIII mean egg-weight begins to rise again and reaches a sec- ond mode or maximum in September, at which peak the mean egg weight is slightly higher than for the period of the vernal maximum (April). This September peak may be called the autumnal weight maximum. The mean dif- ference between the vernal maximum and the autumnal maximum is usually about one or two grams. Having attained this peak of weight, the curve drops again through October to strike its fundus in November (the first month of the second laying-year).° From this point it rises in December to the first weight maximum (hibernal weight maximum) of the second laying year, and then drops again in January to form the hibernal weight minimum immediately preceding the vernal max- imum of the second year. It will now be clear to the reader that there exists a noteworthy circumstance with reference to these curves of numerical production and of egg-weight: they parallel one another to a remarkable degree. The vernal maxima of production and of weight fall together in April; and the autumnal maxima of production and of weight fall together in September. The only departure from coin- cidence lies in the circumstance that the summer produc- tion minimum arrives in August, while the summer weight minimum is found in July. It should be said, however, that the plotting of the curves on ten or five-day _ ordinates might show a closer correspondence of these minima in point of time. The difference observed is -scarcely significant. The definite agreements in the trends of the respective curves are taken to indicate that, on the average, increased production is accompanied by increased mean weight of the eggs produced; and that, vice versa, a decrease in production is, on the average, accompanied by decreased mean weight in the eggs pro- duced. Whatever, therefore, may be the biological sig- nificance of the two production maxima for the hen’s first 5 It has become common to consider the laying year of a hen as extending from November 1 of the pullet year to and including October 31 of the year following. No. 628] EGG-WEIGHT 383 laying year, the weight maxima would appear to possess a similar significance. Since the two are so closely cor- related it would seem possible to measure a hen’s innate egg-producing ability by the one phenomenon as well as by the other. This constitutes the hypothesis which we will now attempt to verify. IT If we take a cross-section of the April production as nearly as possible to the absolute mode® of the weight curve, we learn that, although the egg-weight of most of the individuals of the flock has increased at this time, there are a few in which it has not increased significantly ; and a still smaller number in which there has occurred a loss in egg-weight. The same is true for a cross-section of production taken at or near the absolute mode of the autumnal weight maximum. The following question therefore arises: Does there exist any significant corre- lation between a tendency to manifest an increase in egg- weight at the period of the vernal weight maximum (or autumnal weight maximum) and the number of eggs pro- duced for the entire first laying year (November 1 to October 31 following)? In order to demonstrate such a correlation one must first define more exactly the nature of the second vari- able, namely, the ‘‘tendency to manifest increased egg- weight’’ as referred to above. There must be a fixed point from which one can calculate, for each individual hen, the amount or the extent of increase in egg-weight manifested at the weight maxima. For certain reasons explanation of this point it may be added that by plotting the fre- would vary with different flocks, depending upon the climate, the date of hatching, the method of housing and presumably upon still other varying, environmental factors. 384 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (vorn LIII it was decided to compute all increase or decrease in egg- weight, for each individual, from the mean weight of the first ten eggs laid at the beginning of the first laying year of that particular bird. And, in order to translate the differences into comparable terms, the increase or de- crease was calculated as a percentage increase or as a percentage decrease above or below the mean weight of those first ten eggs. ‘Consequently, the percentage of in- crease or decrease in mean weight for all April eggs, over or under the mean weight of the first ten eggs laid, was ascertained in the case of each bird in the flock; and the same data were derived for the September produc- tion. It is upon the analysis of these raw data‘ that the appended computations rest. In the succeeding para- graphs it is therefore the aim of the writer to demon- strate the following point: that the higher percentages of increase in mean egg-weight, reckoned at the periods of the weight maxima, are so closely correlated with higher production for the first laying year, that, by the method to be presented, a flock of hens may be divided into groups characterized respectively by higher, medium and lower producing ability ; and that this method is effective, whether the computations are based upon the vernal or the autumnal weight maxima. IV We may first concern ourselves with computations based upon the mean weight of the April eggs, including the eggs of the entire month; and it is scarcely necessary to resort to formal correlation tables to demonstrate the point involved. ‘The simpler methods may be employed: (1) Dividing the birds on the basis of annual production above or below the flock-mean and then computing the percentage of net increase or decrease in mean egg- weight; (2) dividing the birds into groups based upon percentages of net increase or decrease in mean egg- 7 It would be impossible to present these raw data in an article of this They will be published, however, at the close of the investigation which is still in progress. No. 628] EGG-WEIGHT 385 weight and then ascertaining the mean annual produc- tion for each percentage-group. To make the matter more clear both methods will now be applied—first to a differentiation of the flock on the basis of production groups. For the purposes of the present inquiry the flocks may be divided into two groups on the basis of the mean annual production which was 120 eggs. One group was made up of individuals whose production was above the mean, and the other group included birds whose produc- tion was below. The mean production of the plus group was found to be 143 eggs, while the mean production of the minus group was 99. After these production-groups TABLE I SHOWING THE PRODUCTION OF THE First LAYING YEAR OF GROUPS OF BIRDS CTED FOR DIFFERENT PERCENTAGES OF INCREASE OR DECREASE IN MEAN Ece WEIGHT, MEASURED AT THE PERIOD OF THE VERNAL (APRIL) WEIGHT MAXIMUM 1 Dirin Sileoeat tor | Number of | Mean Pro- P Sirds Selected for | Number of | Mean Pro- Increase in Egg Individuals | duction for Increase in Egg Individuals | duction for Weight as Indi- | Making the t Weight as Indi- | Making the | the First cated Below Record Laying Ycar cated Below Record La: Y > 10 per cent 2 147 > 4 per cent. 17 132 > 9 si = 3 142 > 3 “ ee 21 122 Se Bn eas 4 141 O Ea 31 122 oa z oe oe 6 140 < 0 of oe 6 1 1 1 E i a aly 9 137 Tee 28 112 a E AE PG 16 134 | Total flock... 379 120 had been established the mean net increase or decrease of egg-weight for each group was computed. The mean increase for the plus group was 5.4 per cent., and for the minus group 2.6 per cent. These results appear to indi- cate that, on the average, birds which manifest a greater percentage of increase in the weight of April eggs are likely to be the better producers of the flock. In utilizing the second method mentioned above, the 8 The flock in question consisted of 38 white Plymouth Rocks hatched in April, 1909. Some of the birds have now completed their seventh laying year. 9 One hen, showing no increase and no decrease in mean egg-weight, and a production of 91 eggs, was omitted from the records. 386 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Vou. LIII birds of the original flock were divided into groups ac- cording to the percentage of net increase (or decrease) in the mean weight of eggs laid during the period of the vernal maximum (April). The percentage-groups were based on the scale indicated in the accompanying table. From the data presented in Table I it is apparent that, on the average, the birds that showed the higher per- centages of increase in the weight of the April eggs were also characterized by the higher productions. Those characterized by a weight-increase of 10 per cent. or more showed a mean production of 147 eggs, while those characterized by a weight-increase of more than 3 per cent. only, showed a mean production of only 122 eggs. The mean production of the group characterized by a . decrease in egg-weight was the lowest of all—111 eggs, this being below the mean production of the entire flock. If the birds are divided into two groups only, one having an increase of 6 per cent. or more, the other show- ing an increase of less than 6 per cent. or an actual decrease in egg-weight, it is found that the high-per- centage group gives a mean production of 137 eggs, while the low-percentage group gives a production of only 112. In this instance the portion of the flock falling in the high-percentage class was approximately 24 per cent. ; and this small group gave an average production that was 23 per cent. in excess of the production of the low- percentage group. The fact is thus brought out that, although a certain small proportion of high-producing individuals that are also characterized as manifesting only a slight percentage of increase in egg-weight at the period of the vernal weight maximum, will usually be found, the higher producers are, on the average, char- acterized by the larger percentages of increase (6 per cent. or above); and the selection of hens on this basis results in the separation of those individuals possessing the highest producing value. No. 628] EGG-WEIGHT 387 y In view of this correlation between numerical produc- tion and percentage of increase in egg-weight when meas- ured at the period of the vernal maximum, it appeared desirable to ascertain whether a similar correlation ex- isted between production and increase in egg-weight manifested at the autumnal (September) maximum. The same two demonstrational methods used in the previous instance may be applied. The data on production were first re-distributed in such a manner as to group the percentages of increase or of decrease in egg-weight under two headings: (1) hens having an individual annual production greater than the mean (120 eggs), and (2) hens having an individual annual proportion of less than the mean production of the entire flock. In this way it was brought out that the plus group, with a mean production of 151 eggs, showed a mean net increase in egg-weight for September of 5.8 per cent., while the minus group with a mean production of 105 eggs showed a mean net increase of only one per cent. TABLE II SHOWING THE PRODUCTION oF THE First LAYING YEAR OR GROUPS oF BIRDS SELECTED FOR DIFFERENT PERCENTAGES OF INCREASE OR DECREASE IN MEAN EGG-WEIGHT, MEASURED AT THE PERIOD OF THE AUTUMN (SEPTEMBER) WEIGHT MAXIMUM, Puncunrace-cuass (“Number of | Mean Pro- || Purcewracn-cuass| Number of | Mean Pro- Birds Selected for | Individuals | duction for Bi Selected for Individuals duction for Increase in Weight | Making the the First Increase in Weight | Making the the First Indicated Below Record Laying Year| Indicated Below Record Laying Year > 10 per cent. 8 143 >4 per cent. — ia | acne 9 141 >3 18 131 E 10 140 >0 y 25 127 bes e Sic Maui 11 142 itt een os 8 108 bh, r 12 141 ea TARA TE EIA 21 111 a. P 14 137 a Total flock. .. 3310 120 When the second method was applied, and the data re- distributed so as to give the percentage-classes, the re- sults shown in Table II were obtained. 10 Four hens included in Table I were not employed in the present com- putations, because they failed to lay during September. 388 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Von. LIII From the data presented in Table II it appears that, on the average, the birds that manifested the higher per- centages of increase in the weight of the September eggs were characterized by higher annual production. Those showing a weight-increase of 10 per cent. or more gave a mean annual production of 143 eggs, while those birds characterized by a decrease in mean egg-weight. showed an annual production of only 108 eggs. When the flock was divided into two groups according as the egg-weight had increased by more than 6 per cent. or less, the high- percentage group gave a production of 141 eggs as op- posed to 111 eggs laid by the low-percentage group. Thus, dividing the flock on the basis of a 6 per cent. increase in the mean weight of all the September eggs, gave a group of 12 hens (out of 33) which showed a mean production 17.5 per cent. higher than the flock average (120), about 27 per cent. higher than the mean production of the low- percentage group, and 30 per cent. higher than the mean production of the small group of eight hens which mani- fested a decrease in mean egg-weight at the period con- sidered. It will hardly be necessary to call the attention of the reader to the circumstance that this method of demon- strating the correlation involved in the frequency distri- bution of these two variables (increase in egg-weight and numerical production) is, by its very nature, such as to constitute a practical application of the means involved. The correlations between weight-increase and produc- tion, considered in the foregoing paragraphs, were so obvious that the question arose as to whether satisfactory correlations could not be demonstrated between these two variables under conditions in which a smaller amount of statistical data was employed. For instance, if the method should prove of value to poultrymen in affording a means for the detection of the higher producers of the flock, it would be desirable to reduce the machinery of computation to the lowest point consistent with valid results. ` It thus appeared pertinent to inquire whether computations based upon the weight of only ten eggs, laid as closely as possible to the periods of the absolute vernal No. 628] EGG-WEIGHT | 389 and autumnal maxima, respectively, would afford a satis- factory basis for establishing the weight-production cor- relations. To this end, therefore, the mean weight of ten eggs laid by each member of the flock between the eleventh and twenty-fifth days of April't was computed, and the differ- ence between the mean weight of these ten eggs and the mean weight of the first ten eggs laid by that hen at the beginning of her laying performance calculated as a per- centage-increase or as a percentage-decrease. It should be added that the production during April was conducted at so rapid a rate that, in the case of 28 individuals out of 37, it was possible to obtain the record of ten eggs within the dates mentioned. In the remainder of individuals it was necessary to transcend these limits slightly. In no instance, however, was it necessary to take eggs from a date earlier than April 8, nor later than April 29. The data thus acquired were redistributed according to the percentage groups, and the results summarized in Table TABLE III SHOWING THE MEAN ANNUAL PRODUCTION FOR THE First LAYING YEAR OF GROUPS OF oe SELECTED FOR VARYING PERCENTAGES OF IN- CREASE OR DECREASE IN EGG-WEIGHT, COMPUTED ON THE BASIS OF THE WEIGHT OF os Eces LAID AT THE ee OF THE VERNAL WEIGHT MAXIM PERCENTAGE-CLASS:| Number of | Mean Pro- |[PERCENTAGR-CLASS: Number of | Mean Pro- Birds Selected for | Individuals | duction for || Birds Selected for | Individuais | duction for Increase in Weight | Ma the the First || Increase in Weight | Making the | e First Indicated Below Record Laying Year | Indicated Below Record | Laying Year > a per cent. 3 142 > > r per cent. 27 | 123 > 7 141 29 | 122 Ss > tt u 9 140 || a o t t 31 | 120 Bean: E 13 138 | wh) e ety 16 | 106 Dal Sha E 19 WE ee o A 7 I eee Sar p 22 126 || | oe 23 125 |i Total flock...' 37 oort Mein the data presented in Table ITI it is clear that the small group of hens characterized by a percentage- increase on egg-weight of more than 10 gave a higher mean production (142 eggs) than any group manifesting a smaller percentage of increase in egg-weight. Each suc- 11 See footnote on page 383. 390 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII ceeding group, characterized on a smaller percentage- increase, gave a correspondingly smaller annual produc- tion, until, when we reach ‘‘<0 per cent., the group manifesting a decrease in mean egg-weight, we find a mean annual production of only 106 eggs. When the flock is divided according as the mean percentage of in- crease is more than 6, or less than 6, we find that in the high-percentage group there are 13 hens with a mean annual production of 138 eggs, while in the low-percent- age group there are 24 hens with a mean production of 114 eggs. In other words, upon the division point of 6 per cent. increase, one may separate about one third of the flock whose annual production is 15 per cent. higher than the flock average and 21 per cent. higher than the mean production of the remainder of the birds. If the reader will now make a comparison of the results reported in Tables I and ITI, it will be seen that the corre- lation demonstrated through the employment of the ‘‘ten- egg method’’ is as clearly established, and as valuable from the practical point of view, as the correlation demon- strated through the use of a full month’s production. VII In view, therefore, of these results obtained from the weighing of ten eggs at the period of the vernal weight maximum, it seemed desirable to ascertain whether the ` same ‘‘ten-egg method’’ at the period of the autumnal weight maximum would also serve to distinguish a group of hens characterized by the possession of higher pro- ducing ability. Accordingly the production data for September were analyzed from this point of view. In explanation of the September results, however, sev- eral points should be noted. In the first place, although September production represents a definite mode in the annual production curve when plotted on monthly ordi- nates, in the case of the flock studied the month’s pro- duction falls considerably short of the April production. In April all members of the flock, without an exception, were laying. In September there were four hens that did not lay at all; and three hens laid only three eggs or less. No. 628] EGG-WEIGHT 391 In the redistribution of the data for the present purpose the records of no hens are included that did not lay at least five eggs in September. Two hens laid nine eggs five eggs. So that, in reality, the results of this case are based upon the mean weight of somewhat less than ten eggs from each hen. In the second place it should be noted that the Septem- ber production was scattered when compared with the April production; and although an attempt was made to secure eggs laid during the latter half of the month, it frequently happened that it was necessary to include eggs laid’ in the earlier part. The results of this redistribution of data and the attendant computations are presented in Table V. TABLE IV SHOWING THE MEAN ANNUAL PRODUCTION FOR THE FIRST LAYING YEAR OF GROUPS OF INDIVIDUALS SELECTED FOR VARYING peenes oF IN- CREASE OR DECREASE IN EAN GG- WEIGHT, COMPUTED ON THE BASIS OF THE WEIGHT OF TEN EGGs OR LESS, as AT THE PERIOD OF THE AUTUMNAL WEIGHT MAXIMUM. PERCENT. ASS:| Number Mean Pro- PERCENTAGE-CLASS: Number of ean Pro- Birds Selec at ror Individuals duction for Birds Selected for Individuals Santis for Increase in Weight | Making thi the First Increase in Weight Making the, the First Indicated Below Record rite Year Indicated Below Record (Laying Year > 18 per cent. 3 147 E per cent. 21 125 be oe A i 6 145 ae | 23 125 po cae (0 Dineen 7 143 >0 Z 26 124 lene” E 8 44 R sks 5 108 ph R ial ek 10 1 EE S 19 112 e TY = adp TOA 2 a Ba 12 139 f Sag 4s 14 is es ee d 6 sa en i 16 i34 | Ditto „plus sta vel Ss 9 te 19 131 ing ponies in tie 12 101 Total fi s 31 120 From the data presented in Table IV it appears, as in the former case, that higher production is correlated with the higher percentages of increase in egg-weight. The maximum group-production (147) occurred in those hens whose mean increase in weight was above 13 per cent. Selecting above 10 per cent. gave seven birds whose mean production was 143 eggs. Selecting above 6 per cent. gave twelve hens whose mean production was 139 eggs. On the other hand, selecting below 0 per cent. (i. e., birds 392 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Vou. LIII showing a decrease in egg-weight) gave five hens with a mean: production of only 108. When we add to these the hens that laid three eggs or less in September, we obtain a group whose mean production was only 96; and when we consider the hens that (1) gave a September produc- tion of 3 eggs or less, and (2) gave a decrease in egg weight, we obtain a combined group of 12 whose mean annual production was only 101 eggs for the first laying year. A comparison of Tables I, II, III and IV thus shows that the last case presents the clearest evidence yet ob- tained for the positive correlation existing between per- centage of increase in egg-weight and total annual pro- duction. The results are more definite than those ob- tained for the ‘‘ten-egg test’’ at the vernal weight maxi- mum, or for the ‘‘month test’’ at either the vernal or the autumnal weight maxima. In other words a test based upon a smaller number of eggs, laid nearer to the absolute mode, gives a clearer indication of innate producing abil- ity than does a test based upon a larger number of eggs laid in a ‘‘seatter grouping’’ about the approximate mode. This conclusion is in harmony with the views expressed by Gavin’ and by Wilson’ to the effect that the best unit of time for measuring a cow’s milk-producing ability is not the year test, nor the thirty-day test, nor even the seven-day test, but the one-day test conducted when the production reaches its maximum. Apparently the meas- urement of egg-production in the domestic fowl, consid- ered as a ee performance, rests upon a similar ` basis. Vil In bringing this paper to a close the writer wishes to have it distinctly understood that nowhere in these pages has it been stated that there exists in the domestic fowl a 12 Jour, Royal Agricultural Society, 1913, 73. Jour, adtag Society, 1913, 5, 309-319. Ibid., 1913, 5, 377-390 (on authority of Pearl). 18 Proc. Royal Dublin Bostely, 1911, 13, 89-113. Jour. back: Agricul- ture, Ireland, 1913, 13, (4) (on aathiorite of Pearl). No. 628] EGG-WEIGHT 393 correlation between egg-production and egg-weight. Most poultrymen believe that, if a hen produces smaller eggs, she consequently produces more eggs; and, conversely, that if a hen produces larger eggs, she produces fewer eggs. This matter has not been considered in the present paper; it will be dealt with at a later time. The point may again be stated, that the significant correlation exists between numerical production and the ability on the part of the hen to manifest an increase in egg-weight at those seasons of the laying year when both production and egg- weight attain their respective maxima. c all riffle connecting two Is; water pass r èg scum; ¢, small rifi from one p another by means of such rifles. d, dry bed of brook exposed during early he tera stages of drought. (Original.) 400 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Von. LIII Attention has been directed to some of the physical con- ditions that existed in a brook water-strider habitat in the early stages of a drought. Certain features of such a habitat in the later drought stages, will now be con- sidered. So far as I am aware, there are no records in the literature on aquatic Hemiptera, that describe the responses of these gerrids, Gerris remigis, after the water in their habitat actually disappears. The statements given here are very condensed records taken, chiefly, from my extensive field notes of the summer of 1911. Late in the summer a trip was made to White Heath for the purpose of examining a number of isolated pools, on the surface of which water-striders were trapped, in the bed of the brook near that place. Particular attention was directed to one pool in which the water had evapo- rated rapidly during the previous week. It was exam- ined about’8:00 a.m. and at that time the dimensions of the pool were approximately 12 X 5 X 4 inches. It was evident that the pool would be completely dry before night. Evaporation was taking place rapidly, as the atmosphere had been very dry for several days. The heat had been intense for a number of weeks, a tempera- ture of 90°—100° F. not being uncommon. - 2. Behavior in a Drying Pool.—There were twenty ger- rids trapped on the surface of the pool, already mentioned. The insects did not move about very much on the water- film. They were very quiet, frequently remaining, practically, motionless for several consecutive minutes. There was no behavior on the part of the hemipterons that indicated any attempt to escape from the unfavor- able surroundings. The members of this species being largely apterous forms and their optimum habitat being permanent brooks and streams of moderate size, with a current of medium velocity, it was, perhaps, not strange that they evinced no responses that showed definitely adapted behavior of a character suitable to cope with such abnormal conditions and unusual habitat changes. At 2:00 p.m. the pool was almost dry, and by 3:30 p.m. No. 628] HABITAT RESPONSES OF WATER-STRIDER 401 there was little to indicate its location except a small area of rapidly drying mud. 3. Hibernation and Aistivation.—A number of writers —among others Uhler (1888, pp. 268, 275), MeCook (1907, p. 265), and Kellogg (1908, p. 198)—on water-striders have stated that during hibernation these gerrids bur- rowed into the mud, under the banks of streams, at the bottom of water under stones and roots of trees, and at the bottom of the pool under roots or stones; and as some investigators, Tower (1906, p. 245), for example, con- sider hibernation and exstivation to be ‘‘fundamentally one and the same process,’’ I was interested to observe whether the gerrids would burrow into the mud, or into some moist, sheltered spot under the banks of the brook, and remain there in a quiescent condition, a state of estivation, until the rains came. However, there were no definite responses of such a character. I doubt whether water-striders do æstivate in the true sense of that term during periods of drought, although more evidence is necessary before a definite statement can be made. As will be noticed later, the gerrids do respond to con- tact stimuli. Generally such a stimulus resulted in in- hibiting locomotor activity and the insects remained motionless with their bodies closely applied to some solid object in the bed of the brook. This might prove to be a piece of dry mud, a stick of driftwool, a stone, or a clump of dead leaves. As a result of contact stimuli, sometimes they would crawl under such objects as have been enu- merated and remain there for a considerable length of time. I have recorded observations which prove that they stayed in such situations from a few seconds to thirty minutes. The gerrids might remain in places of this character for even a longer time, as I have noticed, on a few occasions, that they were still there when I discon- tinued my observations for the day. However, I was not able to find them in the same situation the next day, even after most carefully marking the place. On several other occasions, I have made similar observations, but never 402 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. LIII was I able to discover the water-striders the following day. It is possible that there was a trifle of moisture in such situations, and that it was this, acting as a stimulus, which kept the gerrids there. But during these periods of drought—accompanied, as they are, in the prairie regions of Illinois with excessive temperature and glaring sun- light—I have failed, positively, to find moisture in such places, except under large objects, which were not present in the bed of the brook. Frequently, however, I have noticed that under these objects, in the brook bed, the tem- perature was slightly lower and of course there also was the shade. It is probable, also, that evaporation is re- duced in such protected places. That certain arthropods are sensitive to the evaporating power of air is known from the work of Shelford (1913, pp. 85-102), who has demonstrated that the yellow-margined millipede, Fon- taria corrugate Wood, and the ground beetles, Ptero- stichus adoxus and Pterostichus pennsylvanicus, respond negatively to the increased evaporating power of air. Therefore, lower temperature, shaded surroundings and reduced evaporation may be the three factors that ac- counted for the water-striders remaining in situations of the character that have been mentioned. Of course if it was proved that they stayed there day after day, this would indicate the possibility of their tiding over a short drought in such protected places. I have noticed on a few oceasions, at least, that the gerrids have died, if away from water, when exposed to glaring sunshine and high temperature, during a drought. I also have ob- served, when the water in my laboratory aquaria was allowed to evaporate gradually, until it entirely dis- appeared, that, sometimes in a few hours and at other times in a few days, the gerrids died. This occurred, not infrequently, when the temperature was only 85° F. During hibernation, there is no question about water- striders remaining quietly in one place for a long period of time. But it must be recalled that the temperature is No. 628] HABITAT RESPONSES OF WATER-STRIDER 408 low at such times, and that they frequently seek dry situa- tions. The subject of hibernation will be discussed at greater length in another paper. I may say, however, that these water-striders do not hibernate in any of the situations mentioned by the writers quoted, except under the banks of streams, and then away from the water. The positive thigmotactie responses of these gerrids have been observed frequently during periods of hibernation. At such times they formed tangled masses, which were due to the water-striders crowding closely together. These facts were recorded in my field notes as early as the winter of 1912-1913. It is interesting to recall that Essenberg (1915; pp. 397, 400) has observed similar responses in the ease of Gerris orba Stal. 4. Initial Responses in Dry Bed of oo — With refer- ence to the water-striders in the dry bed of the brook at White Heath, it appeared as if the gerrids might die right where they were, for no movements occurred for approxi- mately ten minutes after the water had disappeared. Soon they began to move away. I can not state abso- lutely what was the stimulus that caused the initial loco- motor responses, although it probably was the total dis- appearance of all moisture, as this was the only evident change in the external conditions. Singly and in small groups they jumped and walked in an ungainly fashion from the site of the former pool. While it was evident that the water-striders were less accustomed to locomo- tion on the land than they were to locomotion on the water, yet they made fairly good progress along the bed of the stream. The evidence that the initial locomotor responses of the gerrids, away from the pool in which they had been living, were due to the drying up of the water is further supported by the following facts: Very frequently, I have observed that when water-striders were removed from the surface of a pool in a stream, or from an aquarium, where they had been kept in captivity, and placed on the ground or on some other solid surface that they at once 404 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Von. LIII became active. This was true even if the gerrids previ- ously had been inactive. They would start to jump and walk in a very agile, if ungainly, fashion, and made rapid progress, especially if they were on a smooth and level surface. It seemed that the transference from the water- film to a solid surface—for all other conditions were as before—was a sufficient stimulus to cause internal changes in the insects, that set free energy in the form of loco- motor activity. To induce this result there was but one change in the conditions. In this connection I wish to direct attention to a statement by Jennings (1906, p. 285) : Often, of course, stimulation does rouse an organism to increased activity. But even in this case the activity is due to the release of internal energy. It may, therefore, continue long after the stimulation which inaugurated the release has ceased to act. Such continuance thus does not necessarily imply continued action of the stimulus. In many eases the specific stimulus to action is only the change of conditions. At this point, it may not be out of place to refer to cer- tain observations which, possibly, may have a bearing on the locomotor responses of the gerrids, after the drying up of the pool of water. Abbott (1918, p. 234), in con- nection with some responses of land isopods, Oniscus asellus Linn., Porcellio. rathkei Brandt, and Porcellio scaber Latreille, to humidity and evaporation makes the following statement : So far as observation shows, the effect on land isopods of exposure to a dry atmosphere, including the first effect in desiccation experi- ments, is an inerease of activity. This is a useful adaptation, provided the activity carries them to other regions where moisture conditions ap- proach more nearly the optimum. ` 5. Behavior in Dry Bed of Brook. Sey ten yards farther downstream there was another much larger pool. Its dimensions were approximately 3 yds. X 2 yds. X 5 in. I was interested to observe how many, indeed if any, of the gerrids would reach it, and thus tide over the period of drought, Of the twenty water-striders present, eight of them went in the direction of the large pool, and wandered in an aimless, awkward manner down the dry No. 628] HABITAT RESPONSES OF WATER-STRIDER 405 channel of the brook. They first tried one path and then another. These trials resulted in bringing some of the gerrids to the. pool, but with reference to other water- striders the trials were not so successful. I found that it was a rather strenuous task to watch all the gerrids, although some of them frequently would come to rest. Without giving too many details, it may be stated that all of the eight individuals, already mentioned, made their way in a blundering fashion to the large pool of water. The first one reached the pool in 5 minutes and 30 seconds, and at once moved on to the surface-film. The time re- quired by the others to reach the water varied to a con- siderable extent, the average being 14 minutes and 30 seconds. The last individual, of the group of eight, to reach the pool was fifteen minutes in making the journey. Three gerrids out of the eight traversed the distance to the pool with only a limited number of stops and devia- tions from the straight path. However, this is not to say that the journey was free from erratic movements, on the part of these insects, for such was not the case. The stops usually occurred when the gerrids came in contact with some obstacle in their path. It was not at all uncommon, when the insects moved away from their stopping place, for the direction of locomotion to be changed. The five other water-striders frequently came to rest against stones and pieces of dry mud. Such contact pauses were evidences of the thigmotactice pro- pensities of the gerrids. They also wandered from side to side of the stream bed, trying one direction for a certain length of time, and then, through the influence of some stimulus, trying another. However, they made only a very few turns, directly away from the down- stream pool during their entire journey. One water- strider crawled under a piece of dried mud and remained there for three minutes. There were twelve other water-striders that have not yet been accounted for. Four of these wandered up- stream and from one bank of the brook to the other, first 406 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII in one direction and then in another. Sometimes they stopped with their bodies in contact with some obstacle in their path. Frequently the direction was changed when they renewed the journey. The gerrids did not turn down- stream for any considerable distance during any of their movements. After having observed them at intervals for two hours, I decided that they were unlikely to reach the water unless they eventually should wander to a pool somewhere upstream. The gerrids had moved away from the site of the former pool to such a distance that the water-strider farthest upstream was twenty yards dis- tant from the starting point. One individual crawled under a piece of driftwood and had been in that situa- _ tion for twenty-five minutes, when the observations were discontinued. Another water-strider crawled among some dead leaves and was there for twenty minutes previous to the time when I left that place for observa- tions elsewhere. Of the eight gerrids that have not yet been mentioned, — three of them stumbled toward the left bank and five of them wandered toward the right bank of the brook. There were various obstacles in their paths, such as small rocks, pieces of dry mud, dead leaves and driftwood. The water- striders frequently came to rest in close contact with these objects—response to contact being a prominent feature in the behavior of these gerrids—and at length two of those, that had moved toward the left bank of the stream, jumped into a large crack formed in the baked mud of the bed of the brook. I observed these individuals from time to time—for at least three hours—until I left for Urbana, at about 8:00 p.m., and they were still in the same situa- tion. I should state here that I searched for these two gerrids the next day and was unable to find them; nor was I able to discover the other four water-striders—to which I already have referred—that had moved upstream. The responses of one of the three gerrids, that in the first in- stance had moved toward the left bank of the brook, have not yet been described completely. After reaching the No. 628] HABITAT RESPONSES OF WATER-STRIDER 407 bank, it began to jump downstream. This continued for a distance of two yards, when it turned, facing the right bank of the stream, and jumped in that direction for one yard until it reached the middle of the brook channel. The gerrid again turned to the left, this position directing its head downstream, in which general direction it con- tinued to travel, until after a number of stops, it even- tually reached the large pool. In passing over its entire route, this water-strider consumed fifteen minutes. It has been stated that five gerrids wandered toward the right bank of the brook, on first leaving the site of the former pool. Three of these turned toward the middle of the stream channel and jumped upstream for two yards, turning to the left, they moved toward the bank of the brook. On reaching it, first one and then the others jumped downstream. After a number of pauses and deviations to the right and left, two of them again turned with their heads directed upstream and continued to jump and walk in that general direction. After several erratic movements and two stops, the third gerrid turned up- stream. The insect jumped in this direction for three yards and then stopped with its body in contact with a small rock. It remained in that position for five min- utes. In the meantime, the two other water-striders had worked still farther upstream. On going back to observe the gerrid that had been resting against the rock, it was discovered that this insect had moved away and was walking upstream. These three water-striders were observed for 1 hour and 50 minutes and as there appeared to be less and less possibility of their reaching the large pool downstream, the observations were discontinued. When the insects were noticed last, they had traveled eighteen yards upstream away from the site of the former pool, from which they originally came. One of them had crawled under a piece of driftwood and had been there for thirty minutes when I discontinued my observations. These gerrids were sought carefully the following day, but I was unable to find them. 408 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Vou. LIII The wanderings of two water-striders, out of the group of five, have not yet been traced. Attention already has been directed to the fact that, when they first left the site of the former pool, they wandered toward the right bank of the brook. After reaching this point, they turned downstream. I lost sight of one of them, while observing some of the other gerrids, and I was not able to find. it again. The other water-strider proceeded downstream, but frequently deviated from a straight path, and often paused with its body in contact with various obstacles. After wandering downstream for three yards, it made a complete turn and its head thus pointed upstream. This occurred as the insect was making a jump, it appearing to lose control of the orientation of its body. The water- strider moved upstream for two feet and then turned to the left, walking in that direction for four feet. The gerrid made a wide turn to the left, so that its head was directed downstream. After much erratic wandering, it eventually blundered upon the large pool of water, having consumed forty minutes in completing the journey. It was a task of considerable difficulty to keep all the gerrids under observation, because, being almost the color of the background, they were very inconspicuous. I have records of a number of other instances, treating’ of my observations on the responses of water-striders after the drying up of several other stream pools, at different times and during different seasons. Some of these pools were at distances of less than ten yards, while others were eleven, twelve, and fourteen yards distant from the original pool on which the water-striders lived. It is true that, on the particular occasion to which atten- tion has been directed and which has been described in some detail, half of the gerrids entrapped on the surface of the pool eseaped—when it became dry—to another and larger body of water by making their way overland. But in all the other instances that were recorded in my field notes of similar phenomena, a very much smaller percent- age of the entrapped gerrids was successful in reaching No. 628] HABITAT RESPONSES OF WATER-STRIDER 409 other bodies of water, after their own pool became dry. I will give a few examples: In one instance, only two of the trapped water-striders out of eight reached the surface of another pool, situated about eleven yards upstream. This was not accomplished until the gerrids had consumed much time in random movements, stops and deviations from the straight path. At another time, fifteen gerrids were trapped in a pool along the bed of a brook that had become almost dry, owing to a prolonged drought. Five water-striders out of the fifteen, or one third of the total number, only, were able to find another pool of water seven yards upstream from the point where they had been isolated. These gerrids made many trials, errors, delays, and turnings before they reached the surface-film. On still another occasion forty water-striders were entrapped on the surface of a stream pool. After the water had dis- appeared entirely, the hemipterons left their haunts, and stumbled along the dry bed of the stream. After much wandering about, first in one direction and then in an- other, six of the gerrids found a pool fourteen yards down- stream from their former abode. The method of reaching the water seemed to be due to a crude form of trial and error. I am inclined to believe that in periods of severe drought, large numbers of apterous individuals die. If the drought is a short one, and there is a certain minimum of dampness under logs, stones, and clumps of dead leaves, some of the water-striders may survive in such situations. In regard to physical conditions of a some- what comparable character, Kirkaldy (1899, pp. 109, 110) makes the following statement with reference to Gerris lacustris Linné: In small wayside pools or streamlets which dry up periodically, it is obvious that the apterous inhabitants will die out unless their habitat is sufficiently near to a larger stream or pond; a severe drought occur- ring over a fairly large area, prolonged sufficiently to dry up all the water within that area, would cause all the species represented in that year by apterous individuals only to become extinct. 410 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Vou. LIIL After having observed the erratic, aimless character of the responses of these insects, Gerris remigis, their ability to reach some other distant body of water appeared doubt- ful, and very largely dependent upon chance. If a larger body of water should be very near to the haunts of the gerrids, then there would be, through their random jour- ‘neys, a greater possibility of reaching such a situation. In any case, their responses appear to be responses of repeated trial and failure, until through many repeti- tions, some gerrids eventually reach the water. Ii. EXPERIMENTS IN CONNECTION -WITH Brook HABITAT AT WHITE HEATH 1. Methods.——I now wish to record certain other ob- servations, on the responses of. Gerris remigis, of a some- what different character from those that already have been discussed. Many times when out in the field collect- ing Gerris remigis, some water-striders escaped while I was picking them out of the net as it lay on the ground near the margin of the stream. I noticed that these indi- viduals generally found their way back on to the surface of the water. Several times it occurred to me to under- take a series of experiments with these water-striders for the purpose of discovering their ability to return to their own habitat after having been removed from it. The _ plan was to place them on the ground at various distances away from the stream and observe whether or not they re- turned to the water, and if so to record the readiness with which this was done. Up to the present, the experi- mental work has not yet been carried out as extensively as was desired, but the results that have been obtained may be of interest. The experiments were undertaken at the brook near White Heath, that has been mentioned previously. The site selected was at a place where the bank was flat, with its surface raised only a few inches above that of the water. 2. Responses When Facing Brook.—Twenty gerrids with their heads turned directly toward the stream, were No. 628] HABITAT RESPONSES OF WATER-STRIDER 411 placed on the ground one yard away from the water. All those that got back to the water succeeded in reach- ing the surface-film in less than one minute. Similar experiments were repeated for four successive times, using different individuals. There was no experiment in which more than two gerrids failed to reach the water. Other experiments were carried out in which the water- striders were placed two, three, and four yards away from the stream. The majority of the gerrids found their way back to the water. All those that were taken two and three yards away from the stream were back on the surface-film within 2 minutes and 30 seconds. Gerrids that were placed on the ground four yards away from the brook evinced a few more random movements than was the case of those nearer to the water. A slightly smaller percentage of the gerrids found the way back to the stream from this distance. Those water-striders that reached the surface of the brook did so within four minutes from the time each experiment began. 3. Responses When Parallel with Brook.—A series of experiments were performed identical to those already described, except that the water-striders were placed on a line with the long axis of their bodies parallel with the bank of the stream. It was observed that the majority of the gerrids were successful in reaching the water. In the case of some individuals, a longer time elapsed than was true in the first series of experiments. But most of the water-striders either turned at once toward the brook, or else they did so a few seconds after the first locomotor. movements began.. In some instances the gerrids jumped, for one yard or more, in the direction in which their heads were directed, before turning toward the brook. Some gerrids were placed on the ground four yards distant from the water. Certain individuals of these evinced some hesitancy in jumping directly toward the brook and there were a number of random movements. 4. Responses When Facing Away from Brook.—Other experiments, of a character similar to those that already 412 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Von. LIL have been described, were carried out, in which the heads of the water-striders were directed away from the stream. As before, a majority of the gerrids were successful in reaching the water. The same distances were used as in the former experiments, but in all cases there was less promptness in moving toward the brook. It also was evident that there were more random movements than was true in the former experiments. Of the water- striders placed four yards away from the stream, a greater percentage, than in any of the other experiments, failed to reach the water. 4 These hemipterons employed two methods of locomo- tion on land, walking and jumping. The more rapid progress was made by jumping. The individual jumps averaged about one inch in length, although frequently they were greater than this. Sometimes when jumping, the gerrids made a complete turn, as if they were not able, fully, to control the orientation of the body, and this frequently resulted in changing their direction of prog- ress. In this connection, I wish to direct attention to the fact that Essenberg (1915, p. 399) has observed a some- what similar response on the part of Gerris orba Stal and makes the following statement: It runs with a jerking motion, making from four to six jumps in suecession and then making a short stop. Very often it turns a somer- sault and continues running without interrupting its course until it reaches a place of safety. There it lies quietly for from fifteen to twenty-five minutes, then suddenly begins its race again. Often during one of these jumping movements, the water- striders struck against some obstacle and when this occurred, their direction of progression was changed. There was some tendency for the gerrids to continue to move in the direction which they already had taken, with- out regard to the position of the stream. In the three series of experiments it is of interest to notice that the water-striders reached the brook with a fair degree of directness. It was only in the series of experiments, in which the heads of the gerrids were turned away from No. 628] HABITAT RESPONSES OF WATER-STRIDER 413 the water, that there was much evidence of lack of prompt- ness in turning toward the brook and of random move- ments. 5. Experiments with Barrier.—In the experiments that have been described, I felt that the sense of sight was the important factor in directing the water-striders to the brook. However, there was always the possibility that moisture, as a stimulus, might influence the responses of the gerrids. Therefore, I planned a number of experi- ments for the purpose of obtaining more definite infor- mation on this subject. My idea was to arrange a sort of barrier, along a limited section of the bank of the brook, which was to be of such construction as to permit diffus- ing moisture to pass through it readily, but on the other hand, the barrier was to be so opaque that the reflection from the water of either sunlight or diffuse daylight could not be seen through it. The barrier first employed was made of roughly interwoven leafy branches of trees. This. was held in position by wooden stakes. It was four feet high and extended along the bank of the stream for a distance of approximately fifteen yards. There were two reasons for employing a barrier of this character: first, the readiness with which the materials could be obtained; and second, the fact that its appearance was in harmony with the general environment. However, it was found to be practically impossible to make it sufficiently tight so as to prevent reflections from the surface of the water from passing through it. A number of series of experiments were tried with water-striders placed on the ground one yard and also three yards away from the brook. While the experi- ments were not sufficiently satisfactory, as a basis for definite conclusions, it may be stated that the gerrids displayed somewhat less promptness in reaching the water than was the case when such a barrier was absent. However, in general it was evident that the water-striders jumped toward the brook with considerable expedition and definiteness. If the barrier had proved to be light- 414 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Von. LIII tight, I should have been inclined to explain such a meas- ure of promptness in moving to the water as due, perhaps, to the effect of moisture from the stream. Of course if the means of finding the brook was mainly through vision, it was to be expected that the gerrids would be a little less prompt in reaching the water, because less reflection from the surface of the brook could be seen than was true with the barrier re- moved. Unfortunately, the movements of the leaves by the wind permitted many bright reflections to pass through the barrier. Lying flat on the ground, with my eyes as close to it as practicable, in order to take a posi- tion as nearly the same as possible to that of the water- striders, I found that I was still able to see the bright sunlight of the hot summer day, reflected from the sur- - face of the water. I planned to use another form of barrier and test this matter fully, when, because of the character of the season, I was compelled to take up cer- tain other observations, and I have not yet had the oppor- tunity to supplement this work with additional experi- ments. ; (To be continued) ONCHIDIUM AND THE QUESTION OF ADAPTIVE COLORATION! W. J. CROZIER BERMUDA BIOLOGICAL STATION AND LESLIE B. AREY NORTHWESTERN UNIVERSITY MEDICAL SCHOOL I. As the result of his extensive study of the phenom- ena of coloration in the tropical reef-fishes, Longley (1917) comes to the conélusion that the colors of these animals are ‘‘correlated with their habits in such a way that their conspicuousness is thereby reduced. There is no correlation of bright colors with special modes of de- fence, and no evidence that bright-colored types enjoy _ Immunity greater than that of their fellows.” The gen- eral tendency of this, the most recent, and in many ways the most thorough, observational study of the question of adaptation in animal coloration that has yet appeared, favors adherence to the view that as a whole the colora- tion of animals is of a concealing, hence adaptively pro- teetive kind. There are instances among invertebrates which seem fundamentally inconsistent with this inter- pretation, because the organisms in question are not colored like their habitual background and because they are for this, and for other independent reasons not incon- spicuous. It is also a fact that in specific instances of this sort special repugnatorial systems are frequently encountered. Because of its importance for the theory of adaptation, this matter is deserving of careful seru- tiny. The natural history of the littoral pulmonate On- chidium floridanum Dall affords a case in point. 1 Contributions from the Bermuda Biological Station for Resea from the Anatomical Laboratory of the Northwestern University Medical School. 415 416 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII In his notes on the fauna of Bermuda, Heilprin (1889, p. 187) described as new the form ‘‘Onchidium (Onchi- diella) ‘trans-Atlanticum,’’ saying of its pigmentation: ‘‘ smoke color or dark olive,’’ in dorsal view. Some years later Pilsbry (1900, p. 503), reporting on Verrill’s col- lections at Bermuda, listed this species under the desig- nation by which it is now known, Onchidium floridanum Dall, adding, ‘‘Its dark olive green color agrees so well with the stains on the rocks that it is very inconspicuous. —(A.E.V.)”’ It is possible, though not certain, from these brief de- scriptions, that Heilprin and Verrill had respectively col- lected representatives of the two differently colored types of Onchidium which occur at Bermuda. No constant morphological differences have been detected upon which to base a separation of these types, one of which is in nature blue-black in general aspect, the other characteris- tically of a light (‘‘smoky’’) olivaceous hue. Some in- termediate shades do occur, but in the field a rather sharp separation of the two groups is always possible. The very dark form is much the more numerous. So far as pigmentation is concerned, it is probable that the blue- black types contain merely a heavier deposit of a sub- epidermal pigment, which is chemically alike in both kinds of Onchidia. Nevertheless, we are inclined to be- lieve that the difference in pigmentation may be repre- sentative of a real genetic difference between the two types, for reasons which may briefly be stated as follows: The two modes of colorization are in many places exem- plified side by side; young and adults of maximal size are found belonging to either group; the occurrence of the - pale form is independent of the seasons, and is found year after year in the same spots; in other places, where blue-black individuals may be very numerous, no lightly pigmented individuals have ever been encountered. Therefore we are probably dealing, not with a temporary fluctuation in color, determined by some metabolic cycle, but with constitutionally diverse races of Onchidium; ? No. 628] ONCHIDIUM AND ADAPTIVE COLORATION 417 whether they be ‘‘species’’ or not is immaterial. A simi- lar phenomenon seems to occur in other genera of this family, for v. Wissel (1898) notes that Plate (cf. Plate, 1893, 1894) collected two differently colored forms of Onchidiella coquimbensis Plate; in this instance the color difference between the two types, found in nature under the same stones, was not so pronounced as we find it in O. floridanum. In an Onchidiella which is very abundant at Bermuda one of us has observed a corresponding sort of color difference. The variation in question cannot be in any way a sexual one, for reasons already given and because the animals are hermaphroditic. IL. Onchidium floridanum lives between tide levels. It -is never seen when the intertidal shore-zone is covered by the sea; during this period it inhabits deep crevices and - cavities within the eroded rock. A dozen or more Onchi- dia live together in this way. Their ‘‘nest’’ opens to the exterior by means of a small opening, usually further ob- structed by the growth of Modiolus, which is almost in- variably quite inconspicuous, although the densely packed small black ‘‘mussels’’ within and around the entrance itself may form a very definite patch standing out clearly amid the olive-brown alge covering the surface of the rock. When the tide falls so far as to have left the level of the Onchidium nest about an hour before, the occu- pants of that nest emerge, successively and in a steady stream, wander some little distance over the rock, feed, and then, before the tide has risen Again, the members of each group synchronously begin to creep back to the respective nests from which they originated. The re- markable character of this ‘‘homing’’ behavior we have separately studied with considerable care, and have dis- cussed in another place (Arey and Crozier, 1918). Here we would emphasize simply the fact that O. floridanum appears in the open, on the intertidal rocks, only during the period of low water.? 2 The experimental observations from which some of these statements are derived will be found described in a report, by Arey and Crozier, on the general behavior of Onchidium, now in course of preparation. 418 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Vou. LIII The nature of the background provided on this rock surface is by no means constant in different localities. Commonly, as in the case of rocks in sheltered places, completely submerged at high water, or on lee-shores generally, the limestone is covered by a feltwork of matted Enteromorpha and associated plants, sun- bleached to an olivaceous yellow tint. This is the usual condition within the sounds (Great Sound, Castle Har- bor), and along the shores of bays protected from the surf. In places not so sheltered the rock may be colored by minute green alge, or by the growth of calcareous green or reddish algæ, with which, as in the former case, many serpulas are intermingled; barnacles (Tetraclita porosa) also sometimes extend into the Onchidium zone, but the prevailing hue, and the color of any particular spot of small area, is largely determined by alge. Onchidium also occurs, though more sparingly, on sur- faces of a still different type. In some places the layer of red earth (clay) underlying superficial æolian lime- stone is exposed at the water’s edge, usually in a small cove containing loose rocks left bare by the tide. This earth provides a muddy layer of reddish brown, over which Onchidia are occasionally found to be creeping. From such tiny coves the direct rays of the sun are largely excluded, so that the in-shore surfaces of the stones are overgrown by alge, green or red-brown, un- bleached; on these surfaces also Onchidium. may be found. | The zone inhabited by Onchidium is also that in which great numbers of the common Modiolus occur. These mussels, as already stated, almost invariably line the ap- proaches to the Onchidium nest, and grow within the entering passageway itself. Except where the mussels have settled thickly in more or less horizontal fissures between the wind-formed strata of the limestone, they occur conspicuously in little groups, frequently not more than 3-4 cm. in diameter, occupying every slight depres- sion in the surface; this is usually the case in situations No. 628] ONCHIDIUM AND ADAPTIVE COLORATION 419 where Onchidium abounds. A typical instance is that of a low-lying islet such as ‘‘ Little Agar’s,’’ in Great Sound. The leeward (eastern) side of this island, well protected by the mass of the larger Agar’s Island, exposes at low water a considerable extent of almost horizontal rock- surfaces, whereon occur sparingly clumps of Modiolus, . which are, however, much more abundant around the ver- tical edges of these slabs. Within the clumps of Modio- lus lie many Onchidium nests, from which the mollusks at appropriate times creep out and wander over the flat, yellowish, sunlit beach-rock. It is in situations of this general type that Onchidium is most abundant; in less freely illuminated places it oceurs more sparingy. It is a remarkable fact that, in addition to frequenting characteristically, though-not exclusively, habitats which during most of the day receive the direct rays of the sun, Onchidium comes out into the open only during daylight hours, and never (in our experience) at night; the cir- cumstance which makes this condition particularly curious is that O. floridanum is at all times negatively phototropic, and in a very precise manner, when tested apart from its usual environment. No more striking in- stance is known to us of apparent contradiction between the findings of analytical experimentation regarding the behavior of an animal and the actual life of the same creature, for the snails when on the intertidal rocks move toward or away from the horizontal rays of the sun with perfect indifference. The cloudiness or brightness of the day, however, has but an unimportant influence, if any, on the numbers of Onchidia which appear upon the rocks. That the wandering of Onchidium into the open is determined by imme- diate physical circumstances in the environment is shown by two facts: No rhythmic periods of migration persist when the snails are removed to the laboratory; on days when strong winds and accompanying ocean yp ay prevent the escape of the tidal water rer the partially enclosed Grea Séund, so that there is no occurrence of ‘‘low water,’’ the idia remain id without much impairment; a small amount of air is sometimes trapped within the rock cavities which they nr as nests and this may in some cases help during prolonged submersi 420 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LIII III. The coloration of an Onchidium is the result of its intensity of pigmentation, the texture of its mantle, and the activity of its mantle glands. An adult measures, on the average, 17 mm. long by 12 mm. broad by 6 mm. high when resting undisturbed, being then oval in outline; but it becomes more elongated during creeping, measuring then about 24 by 8 mm., and PRIORI CIE E EE | 3 | 6 6 AVeTageS. -onoo ...s.s--+----+++| 0 o o TEE | | 4. Responses When Facing Pool.—A condensed state- ment will be given now of experiments in which the heads of the water-striders were turned toward the pool. As before, the gerrids were placed one yard away from the water. Special attention again was directed to the factors of time and direction. Experiment XXX.—The water-strider jumps three inches directly toward the water. It then turns to the right, jumping parallel with the margin of the pool for one foot. The gerrid again turns slightly to the right, being now in a position oblique to the pool, and continues jumping away from the water for two feet. The insect turns to the left, so that its head points obliquely toward the pool, and jumps in a No. 629] HABITAT RESPONSES OF WATER-STRIDER 489 TABLE III TIME CONSUMED BY WATER-STRIDERS IN REACHING WATER FROM DISTANCE OF 1 YARD Experiments Heads Directed Away from Water Time Consumed Responses Number of Experiment Minutes | Seconds | Successes | Failures s a a e A ae 0 20 + De, Ai’ A O E E A E AA E 0 -f | E aa CAD EN e A A E E ee 15 25 Z —- te VL ris. Gas kaa See 0 + ASVI oe a ee 0 50 + | p a a r enoa E E E S E T a OF). as Pe eaea 6. Se eee are eas ie EOE oh as eae Ave 4 2 | 5543s of | 0} Totals, pes A experiment XXVI.. re | 2 30 5 0 IAN verages, omitting experiment XXVI.. r ae ae 1 0 straight path until it reaches the water. This water-strider consumed twenty-five seconds in making the journey to the pool. Experiment XXXI.—This gerrid jumps in a zigzag course toward the water, arriving on the surface-film of the pool in ten seeonds from. the time it was first placed on the ground. Experiment XXXII—The water-strider moves toward the pool, jumping in a direction slightly oblique to its margin and gaining the water-film in twelve seconds. Experiment XX XIII.—The path taken at first, by this water-strider, is toward the pool, but after jumping for a distance of four inches in that direction, it turns obliquely to the right, still jumping toward the water. The gerrid is back on the surface of the pool in eleven seconds from the time it was placed on the ground. Experiment XXXIV.—This hemipteron takes a position so that the body is slightly oblique with reference to the margin of the pool. The gerrid jumps along a straight path toward the water for two feet. Ti now turns so that the long axis of the body is parallel with the margin of the pool. It jumps straight ahead for one yard, when it turns toward the water, arriving at the pool in ninety seconds. Experiment XXXV.—tThis gerrid turns to the right, as soon as it is placed on the ground, and jumps for a distance of two feet in a direc- tion parallel with the margin of the pool. It then makes a turn of ninety degrees to the left, thus pointing its head directly toward the ` water. The creature jumps in this direction until it reaches the pool, - twelve seconds after the experiment began. The results evinced in these experiments are typical of the results obtained in many others not recorded here, except a few which are indicated by tables. It is noticed that the water-striders reached the pool much 490 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LIII more promptly than was the case when they were placed on the ground with their heads directed away from the water. All the gerrids reached the water—as was generally the case in many other experiments of a simi- lar character—with but a limited number of random movements. The only prominent exception to this was the gerrid used in Experiment XXXIV. Usually, there were one or two individuals that displayed this lack of promptness. All the gerrids employed in the six experi- ments consumed a total amount of time of 2 minutes and 40 seconds. The average amount of time necessary to return to the pool was 26% seconds. If Experiment XXXIV should be omitted, it is evident that the total. amount of time consumed by five water-striders in reach- ing the pool was 1 minute and 10 seconds. The omission of this experiment reduces the average time, consumed in reaching the water, to fourteen seconds. These results TABLE IV TIME CONSUMED BY WATER-STRIDERS IN REACHING WATER FROM DISTANCE OF 1 YARD Experiments Heads Directed Toward Water | Time Consumed | Responses Number of Experiment | Minutes | Seconds | Successes | Failures | | RRA EEE on Soar Mei, co eee S tee 25 bo Eaa E A E E a 0 10 Ao o OMEN SS ie ca lee N e aao 0 12 Hop SRAL, eee kas ol ieee h 0 11 + E XXXIV. 0 90 + E Bio. ig E E S PO E e an aa 0 12 + ee Spern w Cues Sees A E pa Wk wala 2 6 es Site we OR e se E we wee ek eed 0 264 1 Totals, pA ERREAREN t XXXIV. 1 10 gx Averages, omitting experiment XXXIV. 0 14 ee ees are shown in Table IV. The results of other experi- -ments of a similar character are indicated in Table Y and Table VI. 5. Responses When Parallel With Pori — Some experi- ments were performed with water-striders having the long axis of the body parallel with the margin of the pool. In all other respects, the conditions were similar No. 629] HABITAT RESPONSES OF WATER-STRIDER 491 TABLE V TIME CONSUMED BY WATER-STRIDERS IN REACHING WATER FROM DISTANCE Tao Heads Directed Toward Water | Time Consumed | Responses Number of Experiment | Minutes | Seconds | Successes | Failures V : i -| Ot a0 ie v ct 15 = XXXVIII e -| 0 17 + EE, UP. Gee PINE aren fete ee mh ene tee | 0 22 + AN se 0 Ee + De titi re a ce leo ees | + $A ho os ae Vegi ee eo 52 6 Katka E S seen aaa E | 184 1 TABLE VI TIME CONSUMED BY WATER-STRIDERS IN REACHING WATER FROM DISTANCE Experiments Heads Directed Toward Water | Time Consumed Responses Number of Experiment . | Minutes Seconds Successes Failures s K ee o 13 + LI. 0 i + DEE iae eas e o eee 0 12 + BY oe a a 0 14 eje PAV e a a a a 0 12 ES LVI. | 0 16 + Totals 6... A l 24 6 Averages.... o 14 1 to those when the heads were directed toward and when they were directed away from the pool. The results were much like those evinced in Table II, except that the time consumed in reaching the water was slightly greater in the majority of cases. There was a little less prompt- ness, perhaps, in moving toward the water and a greater number of trial directions. Occasionally a gerrid did not reach the pool at all. 6. Responses When Not Oriented with Reference to Pool.—A number of other simple experiments were carried out near the large pool in the brook previously mentioned (Fig. 6). In these the water-striders were not oriented with reference to the position of the pool at the beginning of each experiment. Forty gerrids just 492 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [ Von. LIII captured from the surface-film were put into a small wooden box. This was taken to the place where the other experiments were performed (Fig. 6). It was then inverted and all the water-striders carefully shaken out on to the ground one yard away from the water. It was of course impossible to watch in detail every. gerrid, but it was possible to observe how many of the hemipterons reached the water. The majority of them were back on the surface-film within fifteen seconds after being placed on the ground. All but two individuals had reached the water within thirty-five seconds after the experiment began. At the end of one minute of time all the gerrids were on the surface of the pool. Sometimes a water- strider was not successful in reaching the pool. These statements are fairly typical of the results of many other similar experiments. A series of experiments of a similar character was undertaken in which the gerrids were placed on the ground three yards away from the pool. As in the ex- periments one yard away from the water, the hemipterons found the surface of the pool with reasonable promptness and directness. The greater number reached the water within forty seconds from the time that they touched the surface of the ground. In the majority of these experi- ments, all the water-striders were back on the surface of the pool, 2 minutes and 5 seconds later. In each of two different trials, out of a total of six, there were two gerrids that jumped away from the pool and had not reached the water at the time my observations were dis- continued. I believe that vision was the chief factor em- ployed in directing the gerrids to the water in the experiments when forty individuals were used at each trial. T have not yet observed the results of placing the water- striders on the ground in large numbers farther away from the pool than three yards. Nor have I made any trials, either in the vicinity of Urbana or Syracuse, with the gerrids for a greater distance from the water than four yards. No. 629] HABITAT RESPONSES OF WATER-STRIDER 493 V. DISCUSSION or OBSERVATIONS AT WHITE HEATH 1. Initial Locomotor Responses.—It is an interesting fact that, just previous to the drying up of the pool, in which the water-striders were living, there were no re- sponses on the part of the gerrids which indicated any attempt to escape from the unfavorable surroundings. Not until the water had disappeared entirely was there any tendency to leave the place. Soon after it became dry the water-striders began to move away from the site of the former pool. What the immediate stimulus was, it is difficult to say. A change in the physiological condi- tion of the body, which might have been induced by the drying up of the pool, would be sufficient to account for the locomotor responses. Whatever the stimulus was, the gerrids began to walk and jump away in different direc- tions. But as Jennings (1906, pp. 284, 285) has pointed out: . movement in a certain direction is due only to the release of inhibition. The organism moves in the given direction because it is moving from internal impulse, and because movement in this direction is not prevented. This possibility must be considered in all eases. Therefore, it is not always necessary to assume that movement is due to some very recent external stimulation. ‘Whatever the explanation may be, the water-striders moved off in the direction in which their heads were pointed. They continued along the same line of progress until they arrived at some obstacle in their pathway. Such an obstacle might be a lump of dried mud, a stone, or a piece of driftwood. Then they usually turned to the right or left, as the case might be, thus being deflected from their former direction of movement. They con- tinued along the new path until they were deflected again, in a new direction. 2. Role of Trial and Error.—Such responses as previ- ously have been described occurred again and again. The various objects in the path of the water-striders served as stimuli to turn the gerrids aside and swerve them in another direction. First they tried one line of 494 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Von. LILI progression and then they tried another. As Holmes (1916, pp. 157, 158) has said: Where there is “ error,” the organism tries again, and keeps on doing so until it attains ultimate success. This statement does not mean that all achieve success, nor does it necessarily mean that the organism possesses any conscious appreciation of means to an end. Cer- tainly, I do not consider that water-striders have such an appreciation. Frequently, on coming in contact with such obstacles, as have been mentioned, the gerrids came to rest with the side or sides of the body closely applied to the object. This was due to their thigmotactie procliv- ° ities. They remained in such positions for varying lengths of time and then moved forward again, but - usually the direction of progression was changed. Occa- sionally, they remained motionless in such situations until the time set for me to discontinue.my field observa- tions for that particular day. Sometimes individuals crawled under lumps of dried mud, under pieces of drift- wood, or among dead leaves. On a few occasions, a few gerrids jumped into large cracks in the baked mud of the stream bed. Water-striders getting into such places, occasionally remained there, but I never have been able to find them the day following the observation. It already has been stated that some of the gerrids reached the larger pool of water some distance down- stream, and attention also has been directed to the fact that on several other occasions, when water-striders had been trapped in stream pools, some of their number were successful in reaching other bodies of water in the imme- diate vicinity. I have not observed that gerrids ever were succesful in finding another body of water that was situated farther away than fourteen yards. In none of these cases that have come under my observation, have I been able to see that there was any definite response, on the part of the gerrids, to another body of water per se. In many instances, the locomotor movements of the water- striders, in so far as their final goal was concerned, have No. 629] HABITAT RESPONSES OF WATER-STRIDER 495 proved to be lacking in definiteness, precision, and in direction of response. Their locomotor movements were very awkward and they stumbled along the route in a very blundering fashion. Their method, if it can be called such, of reaching the water seemed to be entirely one of chance. They might blunder on to a pool of water in the vicinity or they might not. They frequently took the wrong direction and made many mistakes. A better way, perhaps, to express my thought, is to state that these gerrids pass from the site of a former pool to another body of water by a blundering method of trial and error. As Holmes (1916, p. 158) well has said: The method is round about and expensive, but it is better than nothing. It is Nature’s way of blundering into success. It is not improbable that the method of trial and error forms a large part of the habitat responses of arthropods. It is certainly true that a number of writers have been impressed with the prevalence of behavior of such a character among the members of this group.. Among others, this is evident from the work of Bohn (1903) in connection with hermit crabs. Holmes (1905, p. 106) in describing the behavior of the blow-fly larva, with refer- ence to light, makes the following statement: It may be said to be a form of the trial and error method minus the element of learning by experience. Writing of the trial and error method in the conduct of lower animals, Holmes (1905, p. 108) states that: The lives of most insects, crustaceans, . . . and hosts of lower inver- tebrate forms, . . . show an amount of busy exploration that in many eases far exceeds that made by any higher animal. In this connection the following general statements are of great interest, as they show the importance that is now attached to such a method of conduct among inverte- brates: Holmes (1905, pp. 107, 108) points out that: The rôle played by the trial and error method in the behavior of the lower organisms has, as yet, elicited but little comment, owing probably to the fact that attention has been centered more upon other features of their behavior. It may have been considered by some investigators as 496 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LII too obvious for remark since any one who attentively observes the con- duct of almost any of the lower animals for ten minutes can scarcely fail to see the method exemplified. Jennings (1906, pp. 246, 247), also, directs attention to this form of behavior in the following words: In most if not all other invertebrates there occur many “ trial move- ments” similar to those already deseribed. In many recent accounts of the behavior of other invertebrates little mention, it is true, will be found of such movements. This is apparently because attention has been directed by current theories to other features of the behavior, and the trial movements have been considered of no consequence. Often an attentive reading of papers on “ tropisms,” ete., will reveal parenthetical mention of various “ disordered” movements, turnings to one side and the other, and other irregularities, which disturb the even tenor of the “tropism,” and are looked upon for some reason as without significance and not requiring explanation. Further, one often finds in such papers accounts of movements which are clearly of the “trial” character, yet are not recognized as such by the author, on the watch only for “ tro- pisms.” In the earlier literature of animal behavior, before the preva- lence of the recent hard-and-fast theories, one finds the trial movements fully recognized and described in detail... . Unprejudiced observation of most invertebrates will show that they perform many movements which have no fixed relation to sources of external stimuli, but o do serve to test the surroundings and thus to guide the animal. s Holmes (1905) has recently pointed out, in a most excellent paper, a is really a matter of common observation on all sorts of animals. The fact that such movements are not empha- sized by writers on animal behavior is evidently due to their being con- sidered without significance. In a number of recent papers the importance of trial movements in behavior has been more explicitly recognized. . . . I have made a statement about a final goal, but I do not intend to convey the idea, in any way, that these insects are endowed with even the smallest amount of prevision, nor do I wish to be understood as assuming that because of certain perception, on the part of the water-striders, of the exigencies of the case, they therefore responded with a special form of behavior suitable to meet the difficulties of the situation. But, on the other hand, I wish to present _ the thought that these gerrids, in moving away from their former haunts, may or may not come upon another body of water, if there is one in the vicinity, and that this hap- No. 629] HABITAT RESPONSES OF WATER-STRIDER 497 pens not because of any direct or definite response or re- sponses to the body of water per se, but rather is due more to the fact that many of their locomotor responses are spontaneous ones, modified frequently as to direction and speed, mainly, by contact stimulus, many of these movements probably being due not to some very recent stimulus or stimuli which have any direct relation to the body of water, but that they, more probably, are due, as Jennings (1906, p. 285) suggests, to the simple outflow of the stored-up energy of the organism through > the channels provided by its structure. 3. Role of Moisture Undoubtedly it is true that water- striders, Gerris remigis, are sensitive and responsive to moisture. The fact that the greater portion of their lives is passed on the surface-film of brooks and streams would seem to be sufficiently indicative of this. Then, also, the ability to find their way back to the stream in the spring, having left it in the fall, frequently from distances of three and four yards, and sometimes from greater dis- tances, after passing several months in hibernation, is further indication that they are sensitive to some stimulus or stimuli, the response to which results in bringing them back to the water. That the migration of those gerrids from the site of a former pool to another body of water is mainly an expres- sion of hydrotropism, according to the manner in which that form of response is usually interpreted, I believe to be extremely doubtful. However, it is not my intention to assért that moisture does not play an important réle in the economy of these water-striders. But I do not be- lieve that the movements of the gerrids in the dry bed of the brook afford any definite indication that they are direct responses to moisture. It is very improbable that, during severe droughts and high temperatures, moisture, - diffusing through the atmosphere, from such compara- tively small bodies of water (dimensions 3 yds. X 2 yds. X 5 in. and in several instances smaller than this) as already have been indicated, impinged on the bodies of 498 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LIII the hemipterons in any manner that would be effective in producing definite responses to the source of this mois- ture, as for example positive responses, resulting in the water-striders wandering toward the pool. This is the more improbable when it is recalled that the gerrids were ten yards away from the water, and in other instances, not recorded in detail in this paper, they were even far- ther away than this, eleven, twelve and fourteen yards distant. I also have observed their responses in the dry bed of a stream, when there were mon of water at a less distance than ten yards apart., In this connection it may be of interest to quote a state- ment from Weiss (1914, p. 33): Wingless forms of Gerris marginatus, which is quite common through- out New Jersey, when removed from a pond containing some three thousand square feet of water and liberated at distances of one, two, three, four, five, six, seven, eight, and nine yards from the water, imme- diately made their way back to the water without hesitancy. Of course their movements, which consisted of a series of jumps, were more or less clumsy, but all n in the right direction even though purposely headed the wron When liberated a a distaneh of ten yards, they had some slight trouble in getting their bearings, but after making several false starts, finally wound up by going in the direction of the water. At a distance of fifteen yards, a longer time and more moving around were required before the right direction was located. At thirty and forty yards away, they seemed to lose their bearings completely and moved aimlessly about in all directions. Even at the end of an hour they were no nearer the water. The observations of Weiss were of responses of water- striders under experimental conditions and not observa- tions of their responses under the natural conditions of their own environment undisturbed by any extraneous stimulus, as was the case of my observations. However. it is pertinent to direct attention to certain facts in con- nection with his experiments. It is evident that the re- sponses of Gerris marginatus, especially those individuals that were placed on the ground seven, eight, and nine yards away from the water, differ fron those of Gerris remigis. Members of this species do not make their way to water, from such distances, with the promptness and No. 629] HABITAT RESPONSES OF WATER-STRIDER 499 definiteness recorded in the experiments of Weiss with individuals of Gerris marginatus. I infer from the little description recorded, that the responses of individuals of the same species, when placed on the ground ten yards away from the water, were more of the character of those of Gerris remigis at such a distance from a pool of water. While I have not observed gerrids of this species make their way to a body of water quite so far away as fifteen yards distant, as did Weiss in some of his experiments with Gerris marginatus, yet I am not prepared to state that they can not do so. However, if they are able to find water at such a distance, I believe that the achievement is one purely of chance, or the result of a blundering sort of trial and error. On one occasion, I observed individ- uals of Gerris remigis leave the site of a former pool in the bed of a stream and although I watched them for an entire afternoon, only one, out of a group of thirty, had ‘reached an isolated pool fourteen yards distant, when I discontinued my observations at dusk. On another occa- sion, six water-striders only, out of a group of forty indi- viduals, were successful in finding a body of water four- teen yards from the site of the pool in which they for- merly had lived. I would expect, from my own observa- tions of Gerris remigis, the responses of apterous Gerris marginatus, at distances of thirty and forty yards, to be much as described by Weiss, although I have recorded no observations of the responses of gerrids at such distances from water. j I believe that alate individuals of Gerris marginatus, during migration by flight, find bodies of water mainly through the sense of vision, as is probably true in the case of many different species of aquatic Hemiptera, a subject to which Kirkaldy (1899, p. 110) and other writers have directed attention. Recent work on phototaxis— (Holmes, 1905a), (Holmes, 1907, pp. 160, 161), (Cole, 1907, pp. 382-388), (Essenberg, 1915, p. 400), and (Riley, MS.) —has demonstrated that many species of aquatic bugs respond positively to light. Benacus and Belostoma re- spond to light during migration. In the fall of 1908, at 500 ` THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vor. LIII Mankato, Minnesota, a few hundred yards from a large swamp, near the confines of the city, I observed them for several nights, as they flew in great swarms, around the globes of the street are lights. On the ground, within a radius of thirty to fifty feet of certain of the lights, were thousands of these aquatic bugs, both alive and dead. On several occasions, it was possible, in thirty minutes of time, to fill a half bushel measure with the insects. In the fall of 1915, at Milwaukee, Wisconsin, in the vicinity of Lake Park, between the Milwaukee River and the west shore of Lake Michigan, I observed several occurrences similar to those just described. In these instances, the aquatic bugs were not present in quite such large num- bers as in the former cases. The point of importance here is, of course, the fact that members of the two groups, Benacus and Belostoma, respond positively to light during migration. Comstock and Comstock (1895, p. 132) refer to somewhat similar responses. All these facts add still more emphasis to-the probability that alate gerrids, when migrating, locate streams and stand- ing water by means of vision. It should be recalled that such bodies of water are effective reflecting surfaces. However, it is quite possible that both alate and apterous individuals of Gerris marginatus are responsive to mois- ture at greater distances than is the case with apterous members of Gerris remigis. If this should prove to be the case, it would be of assistance to the gerrids in find- ing bodies of water. Further, it must be recalled that the pond to which Weiss directs attention covered an area of 3,000 square feet, while the pools of water to which I refer were very small in size. 7 It is probably true that many arthropods respond readily to moisture. But there is not a great deal of ex- perimental evidence recorded in the literature, treating of the behavior of members of the group, that presents definite information bearing on the particular’ phase of the subject under discussion. The experimental work that proved to be most nearly related to the form of be- No. 629] HABITAT RESPONSES OF WATER-STRIDER 501 havior under consideration was found in a paper by Drzewina (1908) on the hydrotropism of crabs, Carcinus menas. Because of the character of this work, I sha refer to it and quote from it at some length. This writer makes a careful analysis of the responses of these crusta- ceans to the sea. She noticed that when one of the crabs was placed on the beach that it oriented itself and moved toward the sea, even at a distance of 100 meters. Her statement (1908, pp. 1009-1010) follows: Parmi les réactions du Carcinus maenas que j’ai eu l'occasion d’étudier a mon séjour au laboratoire maritime de Tatihou et à la station biologique d’Areachon, une des plus frappantes est l’orientation du Crabe | pi son habitat naturel. C’est un fait d'observation banal qu’un Carcinus déposé sur la plage sė dirige aussitôt du côté de la mer, celle-ci pouvant être distante de plus de 100 mètres. Il m’a paru intéressant de déterminer les facteurs qui influencent cette orientation particulière. Her observations seem to prove that both orientation to and direction of movement toward the sea were not due to responses to light, to the sight of the sea, to the wind; or to gravity, but on the other hand were due to the mois- ture given off by the sea. Observations were made every day for more than a month, at different times of the day, both in bright sunlight and also in cloudy weather. These facts are brought out in the following quotation (1908, p. 1010) J’ai pu montrer que ni la lumière, ni la “vue” de la mer, ni la direc- tion du vent n’interviennent dans ce phénomène. J’ai fait des expéri- ences, et j’ai obtenu des résultats identiques, aux différentes heures de la matinée et de l'après-midi, avec un soleil vif ou sous un ceil couvert; les Carcinus dont les yeux ont été noircis ou sectionnés se comportaient à ce point de vue comme des Crabes normaux. Comme mes expériences ont été faites tous les jours pendant plus d’un mois, j’ai eu le vent venant soit de la terre, soit de la mer, soufflant dans diverses directions, vent trés fort, ou faible, ou nul, ce qui ne modifiait pas sensiblement le sens de l’orientation des animaux; bien entendu, quand se vent était fort, il pouvait aceélérer ou arréter les mouvements des Crabes. ce qui concerne l’inclinaison de la plage, celle-ci exerce bien une influence sur les mouvements du Carcinus, qui, souvent, se laisse en- trainer par elle et suit, dans la descente, la ligne de la plus grande pente; mais ce n’est pas elle qui le guide dans son orientation par rap- 502 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Von. LILI port à la mer. Jai pu en effet montrer, en faisant marcher des Crabes sur des pentes creusées artificiellement et diversement inclinées, que ces animaux peuvent tout aussi bien descendre que monter les pentes dans leur “ fuite ” vers la mer. Après avoir éliminé successivement divers facteurs, je me suis arrêtée à cette hypothèse: les Crabes se dirigent du côté de la mer attirés par Vhumidité dégagée par celle-ci; il y aurait hydrotropisme. Drzewina noticed the character of the behavior of the crabs after a heavy rain. The peculiarity of this behavior seemed to present additional evidence that the movements of the crustaceans, previously mentioned were responses to the moisture from the sea. At such a time the ground was very moist. Therefore there was no longer a sharp contrast between the land and the sea, with respect to the amount of water vapor given off by each. The crabs did not go directly toward the sea; but some of them moved obliquely. to the right and to the left; others followed a zigzag course, parallel to the sea; while still others climbed a slope and proceeded in a direction opposite from the sea. I will record these very interesting observations in her own words (1908, p. 1010): Plusieurs faits que j’ai observés viennent 4 l’appui de cette hypothèse. Aprés une pluie abondante, le sol étant humide, quand on dépose les Crabes sur da pente sableuse, ils ne se dirigent pas directement vers la mer, comme ils le font d’habitude, mais ils vont d'une façon quelconque : les uns obliquent à droite ou à gauche, d’autres vont en zigzaguant parallélement 4 la mer, d’autres enfin remontent la pente, dans le sens opposé à la mer. Il est évident que dans le cas présent, comme il n’y a plus de contraste assez net entre la mer et la terre, celle-ci dégageant également de la vapeur d’eau, l’orientation des Crabes se fait d'une façon de la quelconque. This observer found that, when a crab was placed in front of a kind of dyke, which at low tide separated two bodies of water, the animal did not respond by moving toward either body of water, but, instead, it took an inter- mediate direction, and walked toward the dyke. She recorded these facts as follows (1908, pp. 1010, 1011) : Voici un autre fait intéressant au point de vue de l’hydrotropisme: Je dépose un Crabe en face d’une sorte de digue qui, 4 mer basse, sépare deux masses d’eau s’éntendant à droite et à gauche. Le Crabe est attiré No. 629] HABITAT RESPONSES OF WATER-STRIDER 503 à la fois par lune et par lautre; il prend une direction intermediaire et va vers la digue au lieu d’aller vers une des bandes d’eau. The responses of crabs living in shallow water differed from the responses of those living in deeper water. When the former were placed on the beach, they displayed a very definite hydrotropism, but the latter, under similar experimental conditions, evinced no such definiteness of response. Drzewina considered such responses to be adaptive in character. She seems to infer that the char- acter of the behavior, already acquired, must be taken into consideration in the interpretation of their present re- sponses. These observations are described by her as follows (1908, p. 1011) : Quand on prend le même Crabe dans divers habitats, on s'aperçoit que son orientation est adaptée aux conditions dans lesquelles il vit et qu’elle correspond aux habitudes qu’il a pu acquérir dans le cours de son développement. Les Crabes de hauts niveaux, ayant à subir de courtes périodes de submersion alternant avee les périodes d’émersion, C'est-à-dire de dessiccation relative, sont très sensibles aux contrastes de Vhumidité et de la secheresse et, déposés sur la plage, manifestent un hydrotropisme très net. Mais les Carcinus des niveaux plus bas, pris sur fond vaseux se comportent autrement: déposés sur la plage, ils se dispersent dans toutes les directions, devient facilement, et, surtout, se terrent constamment; d'une manière générale ils sont lents, peu sensibles aux contrastes de l’ombre et de la lumière. 4. Rôle of Vision.—On the several occasions that I have observed the drying up of isolated stream pools, having on their surfaces trapped Gerris remigis, I have watched carefully in order to detect whether the sense of sight was the principal factor in aiding these aquatic bugs to find other bodies of water. The rôle played, directly, by vision, is probably not of immediate importance during their responses in this connection, except in those in- stances when the ground is flat and level and the gerrids are comparatively close to the water. There are various obstacles that modify the possibilities of such an explana- tion. If there are two or more bodies of water in theim- mediate vicinity, it has been observed that the gerrids are just as likely to move toward the farthest one, as they 504 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. LII are to move toward the nearest one. If vision were the main factor in assisting the hemipterons in finding pools of water, they would be expected to go to the nearest one first. Another fact against the idea of vision being the chief influence in guiding these insects to water is that the dry channels of the streams, where I have made my ob- servations, frequently have very rough and uneven sur- faces, with small boulders, stones, lumps of baked mud, pieces of driftwood, and clumps of dead leaves scattered along them. When the small size of these insects and the nearness of their eyes to the surface of the ground are both taken into consideration, it becomes very evident that the various objects that have been enumerated must obstruct the view of the water-striders in a very serious fashion. Then again, sometimes the nearest pool was around a bend in the stream, away from the gerrids, thus making it impossible to be seen by them at a distance. With reference to the experiments of Weiss (1914, p. 33) it is probable that sight was an important factor in directing the gerrids to the water, especially over the shorter distances, one to six yards inclusive. On a bright, sunny day, it is evident that the glistening and reflective qualities of a body of water must be factors of importance in attracting these aquatic hemipterons. It must be re- called that the pond to which Weiss refers was a body of water extending over an area of 3,000 square feet in ex- tent while the pools to which I have referred were pro- portionately insignificant in size. If there was a gradual slope to the shore of this pond and if the ground, where the experiments of Weiss were performed, had a smooth surface free of obstructions to the view, all this should be in favor of the idea that vision was the important factor in directing the water-striders back again to the pond. However, the local physical conditions are not described. Certain experiments of Drzewina (1908) are, perhaps, worthy of mention in this general connection. This writer found, in her observations on the hydrotropism of crabs, that these animals responded positively, and with No. 629] HABITAT RESPONSES OF WATER-STRIDER 505 considerable precision, to the moisture given off from the sea. However, in other experiments with crabs she con- sidered that the past life of the crustaceans and the char- acter of the behavior, already acquired, must be taken into consideration, in the interpretation of their present re- sponses. Crabs, living in deep water, among rocks cov- ered with alge and beaten by the waves, when placed on the sand, in the vicinity of the sea, did not evince definite hydrotropic movements, but, on the other hand, their re- sponses were of a very different character. The factor, in these responses, of importance to the present discus- sion is that of sight. Vision, apparently, played a promi- nent rôle in determining the direction of movement of the erabs. Drzewina (1908, p. 1011) has given a rather full statement concerning these facts: Les Carcinus de la zone basse de Fucus serratus, pris & une pointe rocheuse (Gatteville), où ils vivent eramponnés parmi les rochers couverts d’algues et battus par les flots, se comportent encore autre- ment: lâches sur du sable, au voisinage de la mer, au lieu de descendre vers celle-ci, ils se dirigent immédiatement, en ligne droite, vers des rochers couverts d'algues, ces rochers pouvant etre sities a plusieurs mètres de distance latéralement à droite, à gauche, ou à la limite d’eau, ou même dans le sens opposé à la mer. Et ceci, quelle que soit la direc- tion du vent et du soleil. Ces mêmes Crabes, déposés sur du sable clair, légèrement humide, où, par places, se trouvent disséminées des taches sombres de Fucus, se dirigent vers ces taches. Jamais je nai pu con- stater, avec ces Crabes, d'orientation directe par rapport à la mer, mais toujours une attraction très prononcée exercée soit par des saroe soit par des touffes d’algues, par des surfaces d’ombre, en un m Ces quelques faits montrent combien il est important, ae Vinter- prétation des réactions, de tenir compte du passé de l’animal et des “ habitudes ” que celui-ci a pu créer. Dans ’hydrotropisme du Carcinus maenas, Vintervention des habitudes est des plus manifestes. (To be concluded) BEHAVIOR AND ASSIMILATION DR. HENRY D. HOOKER, JR. UNIVERSITY OF MISSOURI I In a discussion of Liebig’s law of the minimum (Hooker, 17), proof was given of the existence of an integrating principle which, as Adams (’18, p. 481) points out, is equivalent to Bancroft’s law, so called because Bancroft (711) was the first to indicate the application of Le Cha- telier’s theorem to biology. In fact, if it be admitted that organisms are systems in equilibrium, it follows that they obey the theorem of Le Chatelier. Bancroft’s formula- tion of the law is ‘‘that a system tends to'change so as to minimize an external disturbance.’’ But this statement. is so broad that it fails to convey the full significance of the theorem and apparently has led to some confusion. It therefore seems advisable to give a detailed discussion of the theorem of Le Chatelier in its application to biol- ogy and more particularly to point out its relation to other biological principles. “It will be perceived,’’ says Troland (’17, p. 325), that the demand... is not for new biological facts, but for physico-chemical conceptions in terms of which a chaos of biological facts, already at hand, can be ex- plained or systematized.”’ Findlay (’04, p. 56) defines the theorem of Le Chatelier as follows: If a system in equilibrium is subjected to a constraint by which the equilibrium is shifted, a reaction takes place which opposes the con- straint, i. e., one by which its effect is partially annulled. ... In all cases, whenever changes in the external condition of a system in equi- librium are produced, processes also oceur within the system which tend to counteract the effect of the external changes. 506 No. 629] BEHAVIOR AND ASSIMILATION 507 Let us consider, by way of example, a simple case of a system of three phases, namely, ice, water and water- vapor, in equilibrium with respect to temperature and pressure. This system can exist only at 0° C. and at atmospheric pressure. If heat is withdrawn from the system and the pressure and volume are kept constant, a part of the water freezes to ice and the temperature is maintained by the latent heat of fusion. Since the for- mation of ice would increase the volume and therefore raise the pressure, a certain amount of vapor condenses to water. If heat is added to the system, changes take place in the reverse direction. Similar changes occur when the pressure is altered at constant temperature. In general (Findley, ’04, pp. 56, 57), ‘‘so long as the three phases are present, no change in the. temperature or pressure of the system can occur, but only changes in the relative amounts of the phases; that is to say, the effect on the system of change in the external conditions is op- posed by the reactions or changes which take place within the system (p. 60). If the specific volumes of the phases are known and the sign of the heat effects which accom- . pany the transformation of one phase’into the other, it is ` possible to predict (by means of the theorem of Le Cha- telier) the changes which will be produced in the system by alterations of the pressure and temperature. .. . It should be noted that all three phases are involved in the change.” It is evident that these remarks apply in detail to the behavior of living organisms. The system in equilibrium is the organism. The external condition of the system is the environment. The constraint by which the equilib- rium is shifted is the stimulus. The reaction that op- poses the constraint and partially annuls its effect is the response of the irritable mechanism. A stimulus is generally considered to be any change in the relation between the organism and a factor of its en- vironment, but no response occurs unless the change be one by which the equilibrium is shifted, to effect which it 508 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Voit LII must be, (a) of a magnitude sufficient to overcome the inertia of the system, that is it must be a liminal stimulus and (b) it must relate to a limiting factor of assimila- tion) cf. Hooker, ’17, p. 204). Furthermore, the change in the relation between the organism and a factor of its environment may be produced either by a change in the environment, i. e., it is heterotrophic, or by a change in the organism, i. e., it is autotrophic, or by changes in both. The response is characterized by the facts that (a) its nature and direction are determined by the stimulus but (b) the energy is supplied by processes that occur within the system; in other words the stimulus releases the response. Thus in the ice-water-vapor system when heat is withdrawn, this is supplied by the latent heat of fusion of ice. In this simple system there is a direct relation between the amount of heat withdrawn and the amount supplied. Such a relation does not exist in the behavior of organisms, nor should we expect it in such complicated systems. A wealth of examples illustrating the applica- tion of the theorem of Le Chatelier to the behavior of plants and animals will be found in Bancroft’s article (717). A brief discussion of the integrating character of development, evolution and biotic succession has been made elsewhere (Hooker, 717) and supplies illustrations of these principles. It will be sufficient to reiterate here that cells, organs and groups of organisms form systems as well as the single organism. Correlations and mor- phogenic responses are therefore conditioned in accord- ance with the theorem of Le Chatelier, and it is unneces- sary to postulate the existence of hypothetical inhibiting substances to account for the normal behavior of parts. Inasmuch as the reaction of a system is directed ac- cording to the theorem of Le Chatelier, every system in equilibrium is teleological. The means that produce the reaction are directed to a definite end, to overcome the constraint, and the reaction might be said to take place in order that the system may be preserved. This is evi- No. 629] BEHAVIOR AND ASSIMILATION 509 dently the source of the ‘‘purposefulness,’’ that has occa- sioned endless biological discussion. The living organ- ism, however, is teleological only to the same extent as the ice-water-vapor system. : These being the facts, the essential problem that con- fronts us is a study of the physical and chemical aspects of the equilibrium under the influence of a complex of environmental factors. There must exist some degree of correspondence between this equilibrium and the en- vironment. Comparative morphological structure is evi- dence of this and the same must obtain of chemical com- position. The task before us is not so much to ascertain the ‘‘function’’ of any particular substance or element, as to acquire a knowledge of the equilibrium as a whole, for it is only in relation to the other constituents that the function of any one compound can be understood. The need is consequently for a more detailed knowledge of the interrelationships of the chemical constituents and for determinations of how these relationships are altered by changes in the organism itself. This can probably be carried out more readily with plants, but the problem demands more comprehensive analyses than any that have hitherto been made. Investigations with these points in view are now under way and the remarks in this paper will serve as an introduction to them. A knowl- edge of the conditions governing the change from one phase to another within the organic system is a necessary prerequisite to a more complete understanding of organie equilibria. For the discussion of Le Chatelier’s theorem has shown, that in any reaction all the phases are in- volved, and if the changes, such as the sign of the heat effects, etc., accompanying the transformation of one phase into another are known, it will be possible to pre- dict by means of the theorem of Le Chatelier the effects that will be produced within the system by altering any of its relations with the environment. Adams (719, p. 74) says: ‘‘Irritability may not be causally explained, but it seems to obey these general 510 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII laws in the same measure as causal changes.” A com- plete description is the only adequate explanation. Al- though it is impossible to give a complete description of the physical and chemical processes involved in the ir- ritable mechanism, it should be evident that organisms are irritable because they are systems in equilibrium and as such obey the theorem of Le Chatelier. II When Adams (718, p. 474) says, referring to Bancroft’s law: ‘‘In other words this is a perpetuating tendency, a method of assimilation, of which reproduction may be considered but a special phase,’’ he makes a serious error. That some other principle is involved is hinted at in the following passages (Adams, ’18, p. 474, 475) : In addition to influences which interfere with systems as expressed by Bancroft, there are those which reinforce or accelerate (tend to continue or hasten activity) and do not change its character, but only the intensity of the response (temperature, enzymes, repetition, ete.). By this method also systems tend to be perpetuated and organisms in “ favorable” (non- nord conditions, tend to continue their normal activities. rndike in summarizing the laws of “ acquired behavior or learn- ing” ican two laws. The first is essentially a statement of Ban- eroft’s law of response to interference (discomfort or satisfaction) and the second (exercise or repetition) is that of reinforcement. But when Adams (718, p. 475) states: ‘‘This law ap- pears to be a corollary of Bancroft’s law which is con- cerned with interference or retardation,’’ he is dodging the question. The second principle which Adams calls the law of re- inforcement is the basis of assimilation and the related processes of growth, reproduction and inheritance. That the assimilation of an organism may take place, three conditions are essential: (a) available materials to con- struct the organism; (b) a supply of energy; and (c) the presence of the living organism. There are also cer- tain formal conditions of temperature, pressure, etc., which need not concern us at present. In short, assimila- No. 629] BEHAVIOR AND ASSIMILATION 511 tion is an autocatalytic process; because the end products of assimilation act as catalytic agents for their own syn- thesis. In green plants and the independent bacteria it is also an endothermic process, because the end products of assimilation have a higher energy content than the com- pounds from which they are constructed. As an illustra- tion of an autocatalytic reaction between inorganic com- pounds, the formation of Millon’s reagent may be cited. When mercury is dissolved in nitric acid, a certain amount of nitrous acid is formed. Nitrous acid is a catalytic agent for the solution of mercury in nitric acid, and therefore for its own production. To quote from Troland (’17, p. 337), who has discussed the theory of autocatalysis : e suggestion that the fundamental life-process of growth is the expression of an autocatalytic chemical reaction has been made inde- pendently by a number of investigators. It will be perceived that on the basis of the foregoing theory of autocatalysis, this suggestion þe- i nt e growth of the crystal. The customary objection to this comparison, viz., that a erystal grows by accretion whereas protoplasm increases by intussusception, loses its force as soon as we regard living matter as a complex mixture of substances suspended by colloidal sub- division in water, since there is no evidence that the individual colloidal particles do not grow by accretion. On the contrary, it is almost incon- ceivable that these bodies, which are the real chemical units in proto- to time, actually do coincide in general form with the curve characteristic of an autocatalytie reaction. In other words, the process of assimilation is like crys- tallization from a supersaturated solution by seeding with a crystal. The following quotation from Findlay (’04, pp. 67-68) throws light on this process: In general, then, we may say that a new phase will not necessarily be formed immediately the system passes into such a condition that the existence of the phase is possible; but rather, instead of the system undergoing transformation so as to pass into the more stable condition 512 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LILII under the existing pressure and temperature, this transformation will be “ suspended ” or delayed, and the system will become metastable (that is temporarily stable as long as it is not brought in contact with the new phase). Only in the ease of the formation of the liquid from the solid phase, in a one-component system, has this reluctance to form a new phase not been observed. To ensure the formation of the new phase, it is necessary to have that phase present. The presence of the solid phase will prevent the super- cooling of the liquid. As to the amount of the new phase required to bring about the trans- formation of the metastable phase, quantitative measurements have been carried out only in the case of the initiation of crystallization in a super- cooled liquid. As the result of these investigations, it was found that, in the case of superfused salol, the very small amount of 1X 10-1 gm. of the solid phase was sufficient to induce crystallization. ig et serene of the supercooled liquid, however, can be initiated only by a “ nucleus’ of the same substance in the solid state; . . . it is not brought about by the presence of any chance solid. The following illustration is of interest in this connec- tion. From a saturated solution of anhydrous sodium sulphate, Na,SO,, at 24° C. different compounds may be obtained by seeding with different crystals. If a crystal of the heptahydrate, Na,SO,:7H.,O, is added, this salt crystallizes out; if a crystal of the decahydrate known as Glauber’s salt, Na,SO,-10H,O, is added, Glauber’s salt crystallizes out. In this case both the solution and the heptahydrate are metastable. It should be noted that the seeding accomplishes two things: (a) it induces crystal- lization and (b) determines the nature of the crystals. Moreover since the crystals produced are hydrates, they represent products of synthesis. II Living organisms accordingly perform two processes, behavior and assimilation, and depending on whether the one or the other process is considered, they present fun- damentally different aspects. If we make a cross-section (to borrow an expression of E. B. Holt) through the or- ganism in one direction, it appears as a system in equi- librium obeying the theorem of Le Chatelier. If we make No. 629] BEHAVIOR AND ASSIMILATION 513 a cross-section at right angles, as it were, it appears as part of an autocatalytic reaction. Behavior and assimi- lation work in different planes. The theory of auto- catalysis does not explain all biological enigmas as Tro- land (717) intimates, nor does the theorem of Le Chatelier account for assimilation. However, these planes intersect; the two processes are interrelated in the following four respects. 1. The factors of the environment that constitute the external conditions of the behaving system are the pos- sible limiting factors of assimilation. 2. Assimilation is an endothermic process that sup- plies the energy expended by the responses of behavior. 3. Behavior overcomes the effects of the limiting fac- tors of assimilation and so places the organism in what is, under the circumstances, the most favorable situation for assimilation. 4. Assimilation and growth eventually change the re- lations between the organism and the factors of its en- vironment, consequently producing stimuli to behavior. Behavior is the process by which the organism is able to cope with its environment, it renders its condition as ‘‘favorable’’ as the situation permits, but contributes nothing to its increase. It is the progressive element to which change and variability are due. It is the ‘‘guide of life.’’ Assimilation is the autocatalytic process by which the organism increases and multiplies, but which would soon be brought to a standstill according to the law of the minimum, were it not for behavior. It is the conservative element that determines that like shall beget like; it is the principle of heredity. An organism may be defined as a system that perpetuates itself by autocataly- sis and reacts according to the theorem of Le Chatelier. LITERATURE CITED Adams, C. C. 1918. — as.a Factor in ea Its Ecological Dynamics. . Nart., Vol. 52, pp. 46 1919. open as a Factor in Evolution: Its Ecological Dynamics, II. AMER. Nat., Vol. 53, pp. 55-78. 514 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST : {Vot LIH Bancroft, W. 1911. A ASE Law. Jour. Amer. Chem. Soc., Vol. 33, pp. 92-120, also Science, N. S., Vol. 33, pp. 159-179. Findlay, A. : 1904. The Phase Rule and Its Applications. Pp. 1-313. London. Hooker, H. D., Jr. 1917. Liebig’ s Law of the Minimum in Relation to General Biological Problems. Science, N. S., Vol. 46, pp. 197-204. Troland, L. T. w Biological Enigmas and the Sans of Enzyme Action. AMER. Nart., Vol. 51, pp. 321-350 ON THE USE OF THE SUCKING-FISH FOR CATCHING FISH AND TURTLES: STUDIES IN ECHENEIS OR REMORA, IT DR. E. W. GUDGER AMERICAN Museum oF Natrurat History, New York City ARE THESE Accounts CREDIBLE? Even with the cumulative evidence of all the foregoing accounts, the matter seems almost incredible. Dr. East- man, when working up the data for his Reversus paper, © wrote Dr. David Starr Jordan, and I also have written him. Dr. Jordan, without having had laid before him the later accounts found in this paper, doubts the Columbus stories and kindly gives some data from his own wide experience. He notes that the Remora rarely grows over - sixteen inches long, and, although it holds so tightly that it may be drawn out of water, is so small that it could hardly be used as a hunting fish. He further adds that Echeneis though it grows to a length of three feet doesn’t 7 “sit tight” but drops off the minute ‘‘the shark to which it is fastened is drawn out of the water.’’ This observa- tion of Dr. Jordan’s is in full accord, it may be noted, with the records left by Columbus and his chroniclers that the fish cannot stand access to the air, and hence they affirm that, while it cannot by pulling be dislodged from its prey, it may easily be disengaged by lifting both hunter and hunted up into the air when the former at once drops off. In this connection it is pertinent to give here an observa- tion which I made at Beaufort, N. C., a number of years ago. A shark was hooked off the laboratory wharf and when pulled in was found to have an Echeneis attendant about a foot long. As shark and Echeneis were both drawn up, whenever the latter was elevated above the water it would let go, drop down into the water, and take 515 516 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LIII afresh hold. This was repeated several times, but at last the sucking-fish dropped off and swam away into deeper water. A: shark, which I hooked at Tortugas, had two Eche- neises and one Remora on it. Being rapidly dragged up on the beach, the attendants did not let go until the shark was high and dry. One Echeneis was captured but the other two ‘‘suckers’’ got back in the water, and in many other cases I have been unable to bring the fish out with the shark. Dr. Townsend, however, writes (1915) that he. has taken many sharks while on the United States Fish- eries Steamer Albatross, and that nearly all of them came aboard with one or more HEcheneises attached. Thus it would seem that in some cases the Echeneis holds fast and that in others it lets go. The causes of these different actions are not clear. However, it is after all not a question of whether the sucking-fish lets go when brought into the air, but whether it can stand the strain of hauling in a heavy turtle or large fish. Hence the question is one to'be settled by experi- ment and if possible by mathematics. Holder (1995) has been quoted as having tried the ex- periment but without results since his’ ‘‘suckers’’ would not leave the sides of his boat to lay hold of the turtles and sharks. No record has been found of any similar ex- periments. In the same paper Holder speaks of lifting a bucket of water by a Remora which gripped the bottom with its disk. Dr. C. H. Townsend, in an article in the Bulletin of the New York Zoological Society (1915), describes experi- ments to test the holding powers of sucking-fish in the New York Aquarium. 59 5 53 49.6 > 56 5 53 51.0 . 57 8 55 51.4 3 54 4 50 | 48.5 p 53 5 51 | 49.2 19 | 57 12 54 51.1 n 55 13 55 49.5 . | 57 10 55 49.3 13 57 54 50 n 58 9 56 52.6 ve 18 58 53.1 ve 52 14 57 52.9 = 55 14 59 52.1 as 50 21 58 | 52.9 = 54 22 60 | 53.4 “ty | 54 16 56 L id pis 54 1 56 ber me = 52 15 60 | 52.9 oe 59 8 62 53.0 pi 57 5 55 53.8 ro 53 2 50 48.5 id 50 3 52 | 49.7 = 53 2 53 50.0 Pete eRe A We. 165 Oe head and the average number in the other heads on each of 25 floriferous branches from the same plant, the num- bers beginning at the base. On 4 branches, numbers 2, 5, 7 and 23, the terminal head was defective or worm-eaten, and these have been omitted in the table. On 22 branches of the 25, the primary terminal head is larger than the average of the other heads and the difference may be as 530 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LII much as 9.4. On 15 branches the primary terminal is actually the largest head on the branch. In 3 cases the primary terminal is smaller than the average, and in 10 cases it is exceeded in size by one or more of the lateral heads. It will be noticed that these conditions occur only on branches with numerous heads, where the terminal heads of certain individual cymes tend to raise the aver- age. In fact, on those branches which bear a total of less than ten heads, and in which there are accordingly fewer chances for large secondary terminal heads, the average sizes of the two classes are 55 and 50.8 and with two exceptions (branches nos. 24 and 27) the primary terminal is actually the largest head on the branch. On branches with a total of 10-20 heads the averages are 55 and 51.9 and the primary terminal is actually the largest in only two fifths of the branches. In the two cases with over 20 heads the averages are 52 and 53.2 and the pri- mary terminals are conspicuously exceeded in size by some of the other heads. Since the heads of each cyme differ but little in age, the variation in their size may possibly be due to difference in the amount of food-stuff ór water available, by which the terminal heads at the end of a continuous axis are favored. 2. It has already been stated that the solitary heads and the floriferous branches appear in basipetal order and that those from the lowest nodes may not be suf- ficiently developed to bloom before frost stops all further growth. Table II shows the variation in flower-number correlated with the position of the branch. The table indicates a steady increase in the number of abortive heads from the older branches at the summit to the younger ones at the base. The greatest number of heads are found near the middle of the series on the longest lateral branches, which rise from the middle internodes to a height equal to or surpassing the summit of the stem. But the average number of flowers is re- markably constant throughout, varying only from 50.7 to 52.9 for each set and, in general, reaching the maxi- No. 629] FLOWER NUMBER IN VERNONIA 531 mum among the larger branches. It is obvious that there is very little relation between position and flower- number and the same conclusion is supported by the data from other plants. TABLE II RELATION OF FLOWER-NUMBER TO POSITION OF BRANCH Number of Heads Number of Flowers Branch 5 A Average by Groups oN cae Fertile | Total | High | Low /Average| ' | 1 0 10 2 12 | 46 | 46 | 46.0 2 0 11 0 11 | | Fertile heads 3.2 3 1 16 6 23 59 | 47 51.2 Abortive heads 10 4 1 10 6 17 56 47 51.8 Flower-number 50.7 5 0 3 0 3 | | 6 a we oT ey er | aso | 7 1 8 6 15 | 53 | 47 | 49.5 | Fertile heads 8 8 0 5 5 10 | 54 | 47 | 49.6 | Abortive heads ?. 8 9 Ste 7 6 | 16] 53 | 48 | 498 | Flower-number 50.8 10 0 9 13 22 57 48 61.5 | 11 0 6| 14| 20] 55 | 45 | 499] 12 0 9 11 20 57 45 50.0 | Fertile heads 13.4 13 3 5 9 17 57 47 51.1 | Abortive heads 6.2 14 1 4 10 15 58 48 53.1 | Flower-number 51.6 15 0 oe 19 26 58 47 53.3 16 a pi 1 15 16 57 49 52. ‘ 17 0 4 15 19 59 47 52.3 | Fertile heads 18.8 18 ł 5 22 28 58 46 52.8 | Abortive heads 3.8 ee 7 23 30 60 47 53.4 | Flower-number 52.8 20 | li 2 ibd 20 57 47 52.5 21 | 2 2 15 19 56 48 51. 22 3 1 16 20 60 49 52.8 | Fertile heads 12.4 23 3 0 8 11 58 47 53.0 Abortive heads 1.2 24 0 1 9 10 62 48 53.7 | Flower-number 52.9 25 0 2 6 8 57 52 54.3 | 26 | 1 0 3 4 53 47 27 0 0 + 4 52 46 49.8 28 0 p 3 of BR | SE OE aile bosk 2.1 29 0 0 2 2 52 50 51.0 ~ Abortive heads 0 30 0 0 1 1 Fl ber 51.0 31 0 0 1 1 | 52 ower-number . 32 0 0 1 1 | 49 B-r 0 0 1 1 | 56 Total...| 21 | 145 | 278 | di4 | 62 | 45 | 521 3. The number of maturing heads and the minimum, maximum, and aver rage number of flowers per head in 22 plants is exhibited in Table III. Of these plants, num- bers 1-18 inclusive were collected from a variety of 532 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LII habitats and stations, in shade and sun, and in relatively wet and relatively dry soils. They show in every case a small variation within each plant, but a great variation between different plants, the averages ranging from 29.3 to 52.1. It happens that the plant with the largest num- ber of heads also presents the highest flower-number, but in general there is no correlation between: them, and TABLE III VARIATION IN FLOWER-NUMBER ON DIFFERENT PLANTS Number of Flowers Plant Number Heads Soe Ga ie eo en ay High Low | Average 1 278 62 45 52.1 2 26 43 37 40.0 3 26 41 34 31.3 4 15 38 29 31.9 5 14 36 27 31.9 6 31 40 31 36.4 7 19 45 37 42.2 8 25 39 $2 35.4 9 95 37 | 22 10 62 35 | 26 29.7 11 43 39 28 34.2 12 15 51 | 45 47.7 13 rd 36 | 32 34.0 14 11 45 | 36 40.5 15 3 46 | 44 45.0 16 19 38 34 35.5 17 3 39 35 37.0 18 33 34 26 29 19 126 38 26 32.5 20 98 37 26 32.2 21 61 38 27 32.0 22 74 36 25 32.7 the third highest average is presented by the plant with the smallest number of heads. The last four plants, numbers 19-22, were collected from the same station and grew under similar environ- mental conditions in the usual (and for the region prob- ably also the optimum) habitat of the species. They also -present very similar averages in their flower-number. Others of the same station were also examined and demonstrated that essentially the same averages were repeated throughout the group. | From an examination of this table and from additional No. 629] FLOWER NUMBER IN VERNONIA 533 experience with the plants, the writer is led to the ten- tative idea that two sets of factors, which may be en- vironmental, or hereditary, or both, act on the plants independently, one determining the number of heads pro- duced and the other the average number of flowers in each, so that there may result plants with many large heads (as no. 1), many small heads (no. 10), few large heads (no. 15), or few small heads (nos. 5, 18). Table 4 shows the distribution of flower-numbers for all the heads of five plants. In each case the curves show a close relation to the main or secondary numbers of the Fibonacci series, 55, 29, 29, 34 and 34 respectively, al- _ though in only two cases do the modes fall precisely on these figures. Plant 1 shows a rather close grouping of heads just short of 55, plant 9 has over half of the heads grouped at 28-30, and plant 11 has almost half grouped at 33-35. It is scarcely to be expected that the series will be followed closely with such large numbers of flowers; in fact, Stout has demonstrated that there is no relation whatever to the Fibonacci series in the heads of Cichor- ium Intybus. Since the numbers were determined in every case by counting the mature achenes, the numbers should fall somewhat below the Fibonacci series, rather than above them, because of the possibility of some flowers not setting seed. The plants used for these five tabulations were selected merely because of their large number of heads, which offer better data for developing a representative curve. Plants 20-22, with large numbers of heads also, agree closely with plant 19. A moment’s inspection of the averages for the other plants, as shown in Table III, shows that in many cases, such as plants 2 and 3, they could not agree closely with the Fibonacci series, or that an apparent agreement might be fictitious if based on plants with a few heads only, as numbers 12 and 13. 534 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vor. LIII TABLE IV DISTRIBUTION OF FLOWER-NUMBERS Plant 1 No. of heads... 3- 7 15 16 26 23 30 31 38 24 241315 7 3.2 0.1 No. of flowers. 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 Plant 9 Nos Of bonds: L 0-1 9-510 16 26 FSP 6 4 Bie dod: 1 No. of flowers. 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 Plant 10 NO of Reade? 2° P4616 0 90 0 t21 No. of flowers. 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 Plant 11 No. Of kdn FEO FTT 8 T eo +g 2 No. of flowers. 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 Plant 19 Nó. of hadi.. G3 0 4°15 (1S 12 olosi o sg 1 No. of flowers. 26 27 28 29 30 31-32 33 34 35 36 37 38 SUMMARY 1. The number of flowers in each head is greatest for the terminal heads of each cyme. 2. Otherwise the number of flowers in each head is ein tively constant for each individual plant. 3. There is a great variation between individuals but, in those plants with numerous heads, the mode falls on or near one of the main or secondary numbers of the Fibonacci series. DARWIN’S CONTRIBUTION TO THE KNOWLEDGE OF HYBRIDIZATION HERBERT F. ROBERTS UNIVERSITY OF MANITOBA THe period from 1859 until the re-discovery of Men- del’s papers in 1900 was so strongly colored by the views | of Charles Darwin, and so dominated by the magnitude of his work, that it sometimes seems as though originality and initiative had been abandoned, and as though, so far as evolution were concerned, the scientific world had re- mained content simply to quote Darwin. It is the purpose of the present paper to present the contributions of Darwin to the knowledge of hybrids. To this end it seems desirable, so far as possible, to let Dar- win’s words speak for themselves, and hence, although the paper may seem burdened with extracts, yet, for those interested in tracing the history of ideas in genetics, it will perhaps be of service to assemble such a résumé of Darwin’s work and thought in the field of hybridization. Brought together in such a way, an author’s contribution may be more successfully valuated at leisure by those who may be interested. The writer has, therefore, sought to bring together, in somewhat connected and coherent form, the various views, conclusions and experimental data on the ee gs of hybrids and hybridization, found in Dar- win’s different writings. It is also the purpose of the writer to bein into relief certain fields of investigation opened by him, but in which, so far as appears, there has been little or no research since his time. On November 24, 1859, appeared the first edition of ‘‘The Origin of Species (1), antedating by seven years, the appearance of the papers of Mendel. ; 535 536 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LIII One of the primary questions concerning crossing that interested Darwin was the matter of sterility and fertility in hybrids. Investigators before Darwin’s time had been to a considerable extent obsessed by the species question, which crossing was supposed to solve. If a cross suc- ceeded, or produced fertile offspring, it argued that the parent forms were ‘‘varieties.’’ If the cross failed, or if its offspring were sterile, it demonstrated that they were ‘*species.’’ With the sole exception of Sageret (2), none of the earlier hybridists seems to have formed anything like the unit-character conception, and with the sole ex- ception of Naudin and Darwin, no scientific theory was conceived of which might explain the modus operandi of amphimixis in the case of hybrids. By Darwin, the question of hybridization, while indeed for the most part, taken up more or less conventionally, received, nevertheless, broader treatment. To begin with, Darwin held that the inability of species to cross ‘‘is often completely independent of their systematic affinity, that is, of any difference in their structure or constitution, excepting their reproductive systems’’ (la, 2:14). So that, even as early as the writing of the ‘‘Origin of Species,’’ Darwin is seen to maintain that the susceptibil- ity of plants to crossing stood in no relation to the degree of their resemblance to either parent, and that ‘‘the facil- ity of making a first cross between any two species is not always governed by their systematic affinity or degree of resemblance to each other’’ (1a, 2:16). This fact, he adds, is demonstrated by the case of re- ciprocal crosses, alluding here to the relative facility of making the cross, according as the one or the other species is used as the male or the female. Occasionally he says there is ‘‘the widest possible dif- ference in the facility of effecting a union. The hybrids, moreover, produced from reciprocal crosses, often differ in fertility? (ibid.). Darwin again later in ‘‘Animals and Plants under Domestication,’’ refers to the matter as follows: No. 629] DARWIN AND HYBRIDIZATION 587 Why should some species cross with facility, and yet produce very ` sterile hybrids, and other species cross with extreme difficulty, and yet produce fairly fertile hybrids? Why should there often be so great a difference in the result of a reciprocal cross between the same two species? (la, 2:17). Darwin comments frequently in the ‘‘Origin of Spe- cies,” upon the fact that the hybrids produced from re- ciprocal crosses often differ in fertility, and that while two species may be difficult to cross, there is no strict parallelism between the difficulty of effecting the cross and the degree of sterility of the hybrids resulting there- from. As Darwin observes, differences in the results in respect to the relative ease of making reciprocal crosses had been previously noted by Koelreuter, who found, after two hundred trials, continued for eight years, that while Mira- bilis jalapa could easily be fertilized by M. longiflora, the reverse cross could not be effected. With regard to the difference in the facility with which reciprocal crosses can be made, there may be some fun- damental resemblance between this fact and the ease with which reciprocal grafts can be made, wherein Darwin in- stances the fact that the currant can, although with dif- ficulty, be grafted upon the gooseberry, while the recip- rocal graft can not be made. Certainly the well-estab- lished facts of somatic segregation followed by germinal ‘‘mutation’’—so-ealled, should sufficiently indicate that the behavior of the somatic and of the reproductive cells should not be regarded as being so sharply separated as is usually done in genetic studies. At all events, the prob- lem as to the reason for the relative differences in the re- spective facility of making reciprocal crosses, as well as the further one of the differences as in the case of mule and hinny, between the respective products of reciprocal - crosses, are questions that have been but very little inves- tigated since Darwin’s time, and demand thorough ex- -ploration. Since the advent of Mendelian studies in 1900, it has 538 >. THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LIII been rather conventionally and very loosely assumed that reciprocal crosses are invariably identical in type. That such is not necessarily the case, Darwin’s early observa- tions should suffice to indicate. The problem of the fertility of selfed and crossed plants engaged Darwin’s close interest in forty-one cases belonging to twenty-three species. The ratio of the fer- tility of the crossed to that of the self-fertilized plants was found to be as 100:60. In another experiment to deter- mine the relative fertility of flowers when crossed or selfed, the ratio in thirty cases belonging to twenty-seven species was as 100:55. There is no evidence, Darwin finds, That the fertility of plants goes . on diminishing in successive self-fertilized generations, and no close correspondence, either in the parent plants or in the successive genera- tions, between the relative number of seeds produced by the crossed and self-fertilized flowers, and the relative powers of growth of the seedlings raised from such seeds (1b, 327). Darwin’s investigations were directed quite extensively to the question of self-fertility in plants, a field which bears strongly upon our knowledge of heredity, but in which likewise comparatively little experimental work has been done since his time. As the result of his own studies, supplemented by those of Hildebrand and Fritz üller, he was able to say: We may therefore confidently assert, that a self-sterile plant can be fertilized by the pollen out of any one out of a thousand or ten thou- sand individuals of the same species, but not by its own (ibid., 347). Regarding the cause of sterility or inability to accept fertilization, we are as greatly at a loss for an explanation to-day as was Darwin. As Darwin well states it: The veil of secrecy is as yet far from lifted; nor will it be, until we can say why it is beneficial that the sexual elements should be differ- entiated to a certain extent, and why, if the differentiation be carried still further, inquiry follows. It is an extraordinary fact that with many species, flowers fertilized with their pollen are either absolutely No. 629] DARWIN AND HYBRIDIZATION 539 or in some degree sterile; if fertilized with pollen from another ftower on the same plant, they are sometimes, though rarely, a little more fertile; if fertilized with pollen from another individual or variety of the same species they are fully fertile; but if with pollen from a distant species they are sterile in all possible degrees, until utter sterility is reached. We thus have a long series with absolute sterility at the two ends; at one end due to the sexual elements not having been dif- ferentiated, and’ at the other end to their having been differentiated in too great a degree, or in some peculiar manner (ibid., 455). The questions which Darwin raises in this connection are as follows (p. 458) : 1. Why the individuals of some species profit greatly, others very little by being crossed. 2. Why the advantages from crossing now seem to ac- crue exclusively to the vegetative and now to the repro- ductive system, although generally to both. . Why some members of a species should be sterile, while others are entirely fertile with their own pollen. 4. Why a change of environment or of climate should affect the sterility of self-sterile species. 5. Why the members of some species should be more fertile with the pollen from another species than with that of their own. Regarding the general matter of sterility in hybrids, Darwin comments as follows: It is notorious that when distinct species of plants are crossed they r. This unproductiveness varies in different species up to sterility so complete that not even an empty capsule is formed (1b, 463). It is also notorious that not only the parent species, but the hybrids raised from them are more or less sterile, and that their pollen is in a more or less aborted condition. The degree of sterility of various hybrids does not always strictly correspond with the degree of difficulty in uniting the parent forms. When hybrids are capable of breeding inter se, their descendants are more or less sterile, and they often become still more sterile in the later generations (ibid. With the majority of species, flowers fertilized with heit own pollen yield fewer, sometimes much fewer seeds, than those fertilized from another individual or variety (ibid., 464). 540 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIN As the result of his investigations regarding sterility to pollen, Darwin was able to render at least one service, that of removing the obsession which had so long af- flicted the study of the hybrid question, viz., the variety- species discussion. He says: It can thus be shown that neither sterility nor fertility affords any certain distinction between species and varieties. The evidence from this source graduates away, and is doubtful in the same degree as is the evidence derived from other constitutional and .structural differences (la, 2: 4) The question of the chemical and cytological basis for sterility or non-receptivity to pollen, remains still in part an open field for the investigator. One of the most important questions from our present- day point of view which Darwin investigated was the relative vigor of first-generation hybrids as compared with that of their parents. The following allusions occur in the ‘‘Origin of Species.’’ Darwin comments on the fact that crosses between in- dividuals of the same species, where they differ to a cer- tain extent, give increased vigor and fertility, while close fertilization, long continued, almost always leads to phys- ical degeneracy, and remarks: We know also that a cross between the distinct individuals of the same variety, and between distinct varieties, increases the number of the offspring, and certainly gives to them increased size and vigor (la, 2: 296). Darwin havoughty investigated, as is well known, the comparative relation of the offspring of crossed to those of selfed plants with respect to vigor. I have made so many experiments, and collected so many facts, show- ing on the one hand that an occasional cross with a distinet individual or variety increases the vigor and fertility of. the offspring, and on the other hand that very close interbreeding lessens their vigor and fertility, that I can not doubt the correctness of this conclusion (2a, 2 No. 629] DARWIN AND HYBRIDIZATION 541 Again, from both plants and animals, there is the clearest evidence that a cross between individuals of the same species, which differs to a certain extent, gives vigor and fertility to the offspring, and that close interbreeding, continued during several generations between the nearest relations, if these be kept under the same conditions of life, almost always leads to decreased size, weakness or sterility (la, 2: 27). In ‘‘Cross and Self Fertilization,’? Darwin again dis- cusses the effects of crossing as follows, expressing the view: Firstly, that the advantages of cross-fertilization do not follow from some mysterious virtue in the mere union of two distinct individuals, but from such individuals having been subjected during previous gen- erations to different conditions, or to their having varied in a manner commonly called spontaneous, so that in either case their sexual ele- ments have been in some degree differentiated; and secondly from the want of such differentiation in the sexual elements (1b, 443). A eross with a fresh stock or with another variety seems to be always beneficial, whether or not the mother plants have been intercrossed or self-fertilized for several previous generations (1b, 444). Darwin also remarks upon the greater power of the eross-fertilized plants in his experiment to stand expo- sure, the crossed plants enduring sudden removal from greenhouse to out-of-door conditions better than did the self-fertilized, and also resisting cold and intemperate weather conditions more successfully. This was the case with morning glory and with Mimulus. The offspring of plants of the eight self-fertilized generations of Mimulus crossed by a fresh stock, survived a frost which killed every single self-fertilized and intercrossed plant of the old stock. Independently of any external cause which could be detected; the self-fertilized plants were more liable to premature death than the crossed (ibid., 290). Out of several hundred plants in all involved in the ex- periment, only seven of the crossed plants died, while at least twenty-nine of the self-fertilized were thus lost. With regard to time of flowering, in four out of fifty- 542 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von. LIII eight cases, a crossed plant, in nine cases a selfed plant, flowered first. Darwin broached the view that the increased vigor of first-generation hybrids was chiefly due to the forms used in the cross having been exposed to somewhat different conditions of life. He also contended that his experi- ments proved that If all the individuals of the same variety be subjected during several generations to the same conditions, the good derived from crossing is often much diminished or wholly disappears (la, 2: 270 This statement appears to be an obiter dictum of Dar- win’s to the support of which he does not adduce direct experimental evidence. Again he says: Anyhow my experiments indicate that crossing plants which have been long subjected to almost though not quite the same conditions, is ` the most powerful of all the means for retaining some degree of dif- ferentiation in the sexual elements, as shown by the superiority in the later generations, of the intercrossed over the self-fertilized seedlings (1b, 450). We know, he says, that 1 a plant prepeeated for some generations in ancther garden in the same district serves as a first stock, and has high fertilization powers (ibid.). The importance of this view has yet, so far as the writer knows, to be thoroughly re-investigated under com- pletely controlled conditions. It was Darwin’s view, as the result of his experiments, that the increased vigor of intercrossed plants is due to the constitution or nature of the sexual elements, which condition he took to be of the general nature of differen- tiation due to the action of environment. It is certain, he says, that the differences are not of an external nature, for two plants which resemble each other as closely as the individuals of the same species ever do, profit in the plainest manner when intererossed, if their progenitors have been exposed during several generations to different conditions (1b, 270). No. 629] DARWIN AND HYBRIDIZATION 543 Darwin asserts that there is not a single case in his ex- periments, Which affords decisive evidence against the rule, that a cross between . plants, the progenitors of which have been subjected to somewhat diversified conditions, is beneficial to the offspring (ibid., The fact that increased vegetative vigor in first gen- eration hybrids was also sometimes accompanied by di- minished fertility was likewise observed by Darwin, For it deserves especial attention that mongrel animals and plants, which are so far from being sterile, that their fertility is often actually ' augmented, have, as previously shown, their size, hardiness and con- stitutional vigor generally increased. It is not a little remarkable that an accession of vigor and size should thus arise under the opposite con- tingencies of increased and diminished fertility (1e, 2: 108). In the case of Darwin’s experiments to determine the relative effects upon vigor of selfing and crossing, respec- tively, the data were determined chiefly with respect to height and weight of the plants, which were grown on opposite sides of the same pot in all instances. Regarding the relative heights and weights of 292 plants, derived from a cross with a fresh stock, and of 305 plants either selfed or intercrossed, between plants of the same stock and belonging to thirteen species and twelve genera, Darwin says: Considering all the eases... there can be no doubt that plants rofit immensely, though in different ways, by a cross with a f stock, or with a distinct sub-variety. He emphasizes further, It can not be maintained that the benefit thus derived is due merely to the plants of the fresh stock being perfectly healthy, whilst those gra had been long intercrossed or self-fertilized had become unhealthy ; in most cases there was no appearance of such unhealthiness (1b, 260). Experiments were also made with plants belonging to five genera in four different families. One of the most interesting cases was that of a plant of marjoram (Ori- ganum vulgare). The height of the crossed was to that of the selfed plants as 100: 86. 544 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST ‘[ Vou. LII They differed also to a wonderful degree in constitutional vigor. The crossed plants flowered first, and produced twice as many flower-stems; and they afterward increased by stolons to such an extent as almost to overwhelm the self-fertilized plants (1b, 302). Darwin holds that the inferiority of the selfed seed- lings in height can have been in no way due to any mor- bidity or disease in the mother plants; certainly, he main- tains, no such theory of a diseased condition would in any wise hold, in the case o intererossing the individuals of the same variety or of distinct varie- ties, if these have been subjected during some generations to different conditions (1b, 445) In four out of the five cases experimented with, the in- tercrossing of flowers upon the same plant did not differ in effect from" the strictest self-fertilization. Conclud- ing, he says: On the whole the results here arrived at... agree well with our general conclusion, that the advantage of a cross depends on the progenitors of the crossed plants possessing somewhat different con- stitutions, either from having been exposed to different conditions, or to their having varied from unknown causes in a manner which we in our ignorance are forced to speak of as spontaneous (1b, 302). Darwin’s experiments indicated as in the case of heartsease and sweet pea, that the advantage derived from a cross between two plants was not con- fined to the offspring of the first generation (1b, 305). Laxton’s varieties of sweet peas produced by crossing, as Darwin says: have retained their astonishing vigor and luxuriance for a considerable number of generations (ibid.). Darwin concludes: As the advantage from a cross depends on the plants which are erossed differing somewhat in constitution, it may be inferred as prob- No. 629] DARWIN AND HYBRIDIZATION 545 able that under similar conditions, a cross between the nearest‘ relations would not benefit the offspring so much as one between non-related plants (ibid.). Darwin finally also remarks in general: Tt is interesting to observe ... the graduated series from plants which, when fertilized by their own pollen, yield the full number of seeds, but with the seedlings a little dwarfed in stature, to plants which, when self-fertilized, yield few seeds, to those which yield none, but have their ovaria somewhat developed,—and, lastly, to those in which the plant’s own pollen and stigma mutually act on one another like poison (le, 2: 119). The relative weight and germinative energy of seeds from crossed and from self-fertilized plants, was inves- tigated by Darwin in the case of sixteen species, with the result that the weight of the seeds of the former to that of the latter was found on the average to be as 100:96. In ten out of the sixteen cases the self-fertilized seeds were either equal or superior to the crossed in weight, but in six out of these ten, the plants raised from these selfed seeds were greatly superior in height and in other re- spects to those from the crossed seeds. In the matter of germination of selfed and crossed seeds, the results were conflicting. Darwin, however, discovered that, in general, seedlings of greater constitutional vigor are obtained when crossed by other individuals of the same stock, than when pollinated by their own pollen. n the cases of plants of fifty-seven different species, belonging in all to fifty-two genera and thirty different families, Darwin carried out the most extensive exper- iment yet recorded, conducted for the purpose of deter- mining the differences in size, between the offspring of cross-fertilized and of close-fertilized plants. e total number of the crossed plants amounted to 1101, and of the selfed plants to 1076. As a result, Dar- win found that the plants derived from crosses between different strains of the same species, were taller on the average, than plants derived from erosses within the 546 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Von LIII same strain, and taller in the latter case than in the case of the offspring of. self-fertilized plants. The average ratio of 620 crossed to 607 selfed plants in height, derived from Darwin’s tables, was as 100: 86 From the fact that flower buds are in a sense distinct individual plant units, which sometimes vary and differ widely from one another, and yet, when on the same plant, owing to the fact that the plant has come from the same fertilized cell, rarely are widely differentiated, Darwin reasons that the effects of intercrossing can be explained. He says: The fact that a cross between two flowers on the same plant does no good or very little good, is likewise a strong corroboration of our. conclusion ; for the sexual elements in the flowers on the same plant can rarely have been differentiated, though this is possible, as flower buds are in one sense distinct individuals, sometimes varying and differing from one another in structure or constitution (1b, 444). Hence, he concludes: Thus the proposition that the benefit from cross-fertilization depends on the plants which are crossed having been subjected to somewhat different conditions, or to their having varied from some unknown cause as if they had been thus se is securely fortified from all sides (1b, 444). Darwin comments also on the reversed situation, where changes in the external condition result in sterility, for which he seeks to find a logical connection with the condi- tion induced by crossing. On the one hand, slight changes in the conditions of life are favor- able to plants and animals, and the crossing of varieties adds to the size, vigor, and fertility of their offspring, so on the other hand, cer- tain other changes in the conditions of life cause sterility; and as this likewise ensues from crossing much modified forms or species, we have a parallel and a double series of facts, which apparently stand in close relation to each other (le, 2: 126). Darwin’s view as to the reason for the good effects of crossing was based upon the long prevalent opinion that, No. 629] DARWIN AND HYBRIDIZATION 547 since animals, and hence presumably plants, profit from changes in their conditions, that probably such changes operate to affect the germ cells, or that in some way the germ cells receive an extra stimulation on that account, _which redounds to the benefit of the offspring (1c, 2:155 Darwin appears to hold the ill effects of close fertiliza- tion to be due to the fact that the sexual elements in the different flowers on the same plant have not differen- tiated, while in his conclusion he appears to consider the benefits of cross-fertilization to be due to the individuals involved in the cross having differentiated through hav- ing been exposed to different conditions. Darwin frequently emphasizes the same view regarding the differentiating effects of a new environment. But hardly any cases afford more striking evidence how powerfully a change in the conditions of life acts on the sexual elements, than those already given, of plants which are completely ‘self-sterile in one country, and when brought to another, yield even in the first genera- tion, a fair supply of self-fertilized seeds (1b, 477), and again, . . . we know that a plant propagated for some generations in another garden as res ized by any other individual of the same species but are altogether sterile with their own pollen, become intelligible, if the view here pro- pounded is correct, namely, that the individuals of the same species, growing in a state of Nature near, have not really been subjected during several previous generations to quite the same conditions (1b, 450). When two varieties which present well-marked differences are crossed, their descendants in the later generations differ greatly from one a obliteration of some of these characters, and to the Ses pala of former ones through reversion; and so it will be, as we may sure, with any slight differences in the constitution of ar sexual elements (1b, 449). With regard to the ill effects derived from self-f ase mad tion, Darwin says: Whether the evil from self-fertilization goes on inereasing during successive generations is not as yet known, but we may infer from my 548 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Von. LILII experiments that the increase, if any, is far from rapid. After plants have a strictly analogous result with our domestic animals. The good effects of cross fertilization are transmitted by plants to the next gen- eration, and judging from the varieties of the common pea, to many - succeeding generations. But this may merely be that crossed plants of the first generation are extremely vigorous, and transmit their vigor like any other character to their successors (1b, 438). In this paragraph Darwin calls attention to a fact that attracted little attention for a generation,—viz., the im- mediate improvement due to a cross. Darwin was thus if not the first to call sharply to attention, the matter of the relatively increased size and vigor of first generation hybrids, at least the first to subject the question to exper- imental analysis. So far as plant hybrids are concerned, Darwin’s mind was chiefly occupied, as we have seen, not so much with the fundamental theory of hybrids, as with the question of sterility in hybrids and its inheritance. The general question of what is the essential nature of hybridity, and how and in what manner the characters are distributed in the hybrid offspring, seems not to have come to an issue with him However, among the matters of interest to modern stu- dents of genetics are his recognition of the general fact of the intermediacy of F, hybrids, and of the occasional complete dominance of one or the other set of parental characters, together with the phenomena which he terms ‘‘reversion.’’ Regarding the former matter he remarks: There are certain hybrids which, instead of having, as is usual, an intermediate character between their two parents, always closely re- semble one of them (1, 2:15). In regard to the behavior of characters in crosses, while admitting that, in the majority of cases, the hybrid off- spring are intermediate between their parents, he recog- nized that certain characters are incapable of fusion. No. 629] ` DARWIN AND HYBRIDIZATION 549 When two breeds are crossed, their characters usually become inti- mately fused together, but some characters refuse to blend, and are transmitted in an unmodified state, either from both parents or from one (le, 2:67). As cases in point, Darwin cites the crossing of gray and white mice, the offspring being pure white or gray, but not intermediate, and the crossing of white, black and fawn-colored Angora rabbits, in which the colors are sep- arately inherited, and not combined in the same animal. The non-intermediate character of the inheritance in the case of turnspit dogs and ancon sheep is referred to, as is also the inheritance in the case of tail-less, horn-less breeds. Similar results in the case of stocks, toad-flax and sweet peas are cited (1b, p. Darwin (le, 44-45), in discussing what he called ‘‘pre- potency,” was dealing in very many cases with that which we now recognize as simple dominance. For ex- ample, in the crossing of snap-dragons, Darwin found that when the normal or irregular-flowered type was crossed with the abnormal or regular-flowered type, the former prevails in the first generation to the exclusion of the latter. These 127 hybrid plants self-fertilized, yielded in the second generation irregular to regular plants in the ratio of 88 to 37. This is very close to the exact 3:1 ratio which would be represented by the numbers 85:42. Dar- win, however, simply regards it as a _ good instanee of the wide difference between the inheritance of a char- acter and the power of transmitting it to the crossed offspring (1b, 45). Darwin was thus quite unable, with the information then available, to frame a satisfactory explanation for the - various phenomena —_— under the name of ‘‘pre- potency.’’ He makes one remark akve to prepotency, however, that slightly grazes the present-day presence and absence theory of Mendelian inheritance. 550 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LHT We can seldom tell what makes one race or species prepotent over another; but it sometimes depends on the same character being present and visible in one parent, and latent or potentially present in the other (1c, 2: 58) The matter of sex-linked characters did not escape Dar- win’s observation, alluding to cases where a son does not inherit a character directly from his father, or transmit it directly to his son, but receives it by transmission from a mother who does not show it, and transmits it through his non-affected daughter. Darwin observes: We thus learn that transmission and development are distinct powers (ibid.) Respecting the matter of reversion, or what we should now call recombination after segregation, Darwin’s utter- ances are remarkable, especially in ‘‘ Animals and Plants under Domestication.’’ In most cases he regards ‘‘re- version’’ as the coming to light of a ‘‘latent’’ character, as, €. g., hornless breeds of cattle possess a latent capacity to reproduce horns, yet when crossed with horned breeds they do not invariably produce offspring bearing horns (le, 2:44). Darwin deserves credit for strictly contesting the point of view then widely current, that the longer a character is handed down by a breed, the more fully it will be con- tinued in transmission. Discussing some of the cases, he says (1c, 2:37): 2 In none of these nor in the following cases, does there appear to be any relation between the force with which a character is transmitted and the length of time during which it has been transmitted. The basis for such a view, that the longer a breed is handled and the more it is selected, the more homozygous it becomes, was not scientifically known in Darwin’s time, but Darwin actually perceived that the mere repeated act of selection itself, whatever else might be involved, would No. 629] : DARWIN AND HYBRIDIZATION 551 not increase the potency of transmission, or eliminate be- yond question the liability to reversion. Darwin considered it doubtful whether, as was then popularly supposed, the length of time during which a character had been inherited, had any influence on its fixedness, and concluded from the fact that when wild species which have remained so for ages, are brought into cultivation, they immediately begin to vary, that no char- acter by long inheritance can be considered as absolutely fixed (1c, 2:56). In this work, more than elsewhere, Darwin devoted himself particularly to the question of the meaning of inheritance in hybrids. The question always demanding explanation was the reason for the reappearance after the first generation of a hybrid of a parental, or even of an ancestral form, a phenomenon then called ‘‘rever- sion,” including, as Darwin says: all cases in which an individual with some distinguishable character, a race or species, has, at some former period been crossed, an el acter derived from his cross, after having disappeared during one or several generations, suddenly reappears (le, 2:2). Darwin, at the outset, merely comments on the result of crossing as follows: In considering the final result of the commingling of two or more breeds, we must not forget that the act of crossing in itself tends to bring back long-lost characters not proper to the immediate parent form (le, 2: 64 It was noticed that from three to eight generations were usually required before a breed derived from a cross comes to be considered free from danger of rever- sion. What constituted the machinery to bring about re- version remained, but for Mendel’s as yet undiscovered researches, absolutely unknown. The state of knowledge in that regard is well exemplified by Darwin’s remark, That the act of crossing in itself gives an impulse towards reversion, as shown by the reappearance of long-lost characters, has never, I believe been hitherto proved (1e, 2:13). 552 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII Darwin recognized, as did most of the breeders before Mendel, that As a general rule, crossed offspring in the first generation are nearly intermediate between their parents, but the grandchildren and succeed- ing generations continually revert in a greater or lesser degree, to one or both of their progenitors (1e, 2:22). From eases of intermediacy, Darwin proceeds to dis- cuss what we should call cases of dominance, and finally eases in which the offspring in the first generation are neither intermediate nor uni-parental in type, but in which there is vegetative splitting, or somatic segre- gation: In which differently colored flowers borne on the same root resemble both parents, . . . and those in which the same flower or fruit is striped or blotched with the two parental colors, or bears a single stripe of the color or other characteristic quality of one of the parent forms (le, 2: 69). It is interesting to see how Darwin now undertook, in the absence of experimental evidence, to devise a scien- tific solution for the reappearance of parental characters in the second generation of the offspring. Taking Nau- din’s idea of segregation or ‘‘disjunction’’ of the ele- ments of the species, he concludes as follows: If . . . pollen which included the elements of one species happened to unite with ovules including the elements of the other species, the intermediate or hybrid state would still be retained, and there would be no reversion. But it would, as I suspect, be more correct to say that the elements of both parent species exist in every hybrid in a double state, namely, blended together and completely separated (le, 2:23). The above paragraph comes more nearly being a state- ment of the true nature of the hybrid or heterozygote con- dition as Mendel’s analysis has revealed it, than any other account hitherto published. Combining this with the following statements, we have very nearly the same idea which Mendel’s theory in- No. 629] DARWIN AND HYBRIDIZATION 553 volves, based, however, upon Darwin’s theory of pan- genesis, whereby each cell was supposed to throw off ‘‘vemmules’’ which carried the characters to the repro- ductive cells. He says: The tendency to reversion is often induced by a change of conditions, and in the plainest manner by crossing. Crossed forms of the firs generation are generally nearly intermediate in character between their two parents, but in the next generation the offspring commonly revert to one or both of their grandparents, and occasionally to more remote ancestors (le, 2: 383). Darwin then assumes that in the hybrid there exist two kinds of ‘‘gemmules’’ or character-carriers; viz., pure gemmules from each of the two parent forms, and combined or hybridized gemmules as well, and proceeds in the following statement, to give about as clear an ac- count as we have to-day, of the cause for the reappear- ance of the parental or homozygote forms. . .. when two hybrids pair, the combination of pure gemmules de- rived from the one hybrid with the pure gemmules of the same parts derived from the other would necessarily lead to complete reversion of — character, and it is perhaps not too bold a supposition that unmodified and undeteriorated gemmules of the same nature would be especially apt to combine. Pure gemmules in combination with hybridized gemmules would lead to partial reversion, and lastly, hybridized gemmules derived from both parent-hybrids, would simply reproduce the original hybrid form. All these eases and degrees of reversion incessantly occur (le, 2: 383). The latter statement is virtually a statement of the con- dition of things in heterozygosis, in principle as we have it to-day. If we assume the ‘‘hybridized’’ gemmules to represent the ‘‘Dr.’’ combination, we have the necessary substitution. Darwin’s theory was a natural corrollary to his doctrine of pangenesis. It seems strange that with Naudin’s idea of disjunction in hand, and with the phenomenon of segre- gation in peas, noticed by five observers, all of whose ex- periments Darwin remarks upon, that = did not 554 = THE AMERICAN NATURALIST [Vou. LIII himself perform Mendel’s experiment. However, it is a manner of special interest that à priori, in the absence of experimental data, he should have come as near the prin- ` ciple of the Mendelian explanation as the ane passages indicate. BIBLIOGRAPHY 1. Darwin, Charles The Origin of Species by Means of Natural Selection, or the A of Favored rome in the Struggle for Life. Lon don ed., New York, b. 1877. The pees ‘of ee are fae fertilization in the Vegetable Kingdom. New c. 1900. The Vailition a aan and Plants under Domestication. 2d ed., New York. 2. Sageret, Augustin. 1826. Considérations sur la production des hybrides, or variantes, et étés en général, et sur celles des Cucurbitaceés en par- ticulier. Annales des Sciences Naturelles, nm priest SHORTER ARTICLES AND DISCUSSION DOES EVOLUTION OCCUR EXCLUSIVELY BY LOSS OF GENETIC FACTORS? In an extremely interesting article, Professor Duerden’ has re- cently discussed certain aspects of evolution in the light of obser- vations on ostrich farming. He shows that as regards most char- acters the germ plasm of the ostrich is remarkably stable and yet that quantitative variation as regards wing and toe characters is occurring and is being utilized, in particular for a gradual ameli- oration of the valued plume characters. He believes that the quantitative variation in question has a factorial genetic basis, a view which I see no reason to question. He holds that repeated selection may probably extend the existing range of variation downward, but not upward. In this last conclusion I can not ` concur. It rests, I believe, on too close adherence to the ‘‘pres- ence-absence hypothesis.’’ It ‘assumes that minus variation occurs only by loss of factors and further that factors once lost ean not be recovered. I do not think that either of these assump- tions will bear critical examination. Morgan has recorded, in Drosophila, the occurrence of a reversed mutation by which col- ored eyes were recovered in a white-eyed race, and on this ground has questioned the validity of the entire presence-absence hy- pothesis. I have found that in the piebald patterns of rats and rabbits steady progress may be made by repeated selection in changing the racial average either in a plus or in a minus direc- tion. Genetic changes affecting the extent of the. pigmented areas are clearly of frequent occurrence in such cases, precisely as they are in the case of number of remiges in the ostrich wing, but there is no indication that the changes are exclusively in a minus direction, as Duerden assumes them to be in the ostrich. e has observed variation in the number of plumes on the ostrich wing ranging from 33 to 42. He assumes that the variation can probably be carried below 33 by selection, through cumulation of loss variations by dropping out of factors, but that variation in the opposite direction is not to be expected because 42 is the present maximum and factors for a higher number having once been lost can not be recovered. Of course, the thing to do in order to test the validity of this view is to give it an experimental trial, 1 Duerden, J. E., ‘‘The Germ Plasm of the Ostrich,’? Amer. NAT., 53, p. 312 555 556 THE AMERICAN NATURALIST (Vou. LIL and this, no doubt, Duerden is already doing. If the 42-plumed cock has descendants with a higher plume number than 42, the theory will have been disproved, which would undoubtly be highly pleasing to Duerden because it would give him a more hopeful basis for economic work. Now my own experimental work with loss-variations leads me strongly to hold the more hopeful view, that genetic changes are plus as well as minus, even in the case of structures which are in course of phylogenetic de- generation. The degenerating lateral digits of the guinea-pig’s foot? pre- sent a case parallel with those of the degenerating wing and the degenerating fourth toe of the ostrich. The guinea-pig, like all wild species of the genus, Cavia, has lost altogether the first of the five typical digits, and has lost the fifth digit from its hind foot, but not from the front foot. Some years ago I discovered a guinea-pig which had an imperfectly developed fifth digit on one hind foot. Neither of its parents had a fifth digit on either hind foot. This fact ‘alone shows the possibility of plus fluctuation in a degenerate organ. The polydactylous individual, a male, was mated both with related and with unrelated females. By the former, he had 13 polydactylous and 32 normal individuals; by the latter he had 2 polydactylous and 30 normal individuals. This result showed that normal females related to the polydactyl male, even though themselves normal, transmitted a factor or factors favorable to the production of the fifth toe, since more of their offspring were polydactyl than of the offspring of ordinary females, when both sorts were mated to the same polydactyl male. Breeding the polydactyl offspring together and continuing the race by selecting these individuals which had the best developed toes (purely somatic selection), a race was secured within four generations which produced regularly 90 to 100 per cent. of polydactylous young. The race was continued for several years and showed no signs during this period of returning deterioration. In this case we have an example of plus fluctuation in a char- acter supposed to have been completely lost from the genus, Cavia, yet which, having shown itself sporadically and feebly in a single individual, was recovered and fully established as a racial char- acter by the practise of inbreeding and selection on a purely somatic basis. The first digit has, so far as I know, never been observed to 2 Cas tle, W. E., ‘