Hi iiil 1 l!il I Hlili'HiHMM! MiifflHI fjji[fi|(riumiiiii»n lifin uuiiii siwn mn i Uiii'n HU(iJ";m!(|tu:ftM!f!n m MHIH IH|»I I Sin tnft hits!)* fummtn will mn t It Jlltl II JUMI I HHffi IP ill wM iiniui itt?i[?iutUUfiH irdH inihiiHtTiltitiuHiuii ntt i i huini hluiHuihniHJM utii i mlljid tutu 'Mi ut'iuh'it I'lMIHiffl II if«li?jl| I. iiil II f ?xijHi HUH ■— WW tlUIHHWilllU' nunansiPiiHiiii' '.lit miiin ;iitiiiiU!iii»!imn iihm ?!iHiunuii mil itui HHUl>!ttl!rMiii:;n;!,i! in-'ij'uii) hi ii h few \\Um \\mhm\um ffififUiUtkl hUMMm i«m in HhsinihUdnihiitin m mm iff 6/2- GIFT OF TO THE Library of the Medical Department of THE University of California rt*^ MEDICAL SCHOOL LIB1RARY 1 A TEXT- BOO //^•^ & T£*T1BOO<< or f^^^^JJm, M mmm eJicai liepa^ *^ OF PHYSIOLOGY FOR MEDICAL STUDENTS AND PHYSICIANS BY WILLIAM H. HOWELL, Ph.D., M.D., LL.D. PROFESSOR OF PHYSIOLOGY IN THE JOHNS HOPKINS UNIVERSITY, BALTIMORE i ' ! « ,ff:iiust vat**V PHILADELPHIA AND LONDON W. B. SAUNDERS COMPANY 1 000 Set up, electrotyped, printed and copyrighted, September, 1905. Copyright, 1905, by W. B. Saunders & Company. Reprinted, February, 1906. PRESS 0«= W. B. SAUNDERS COMPANY, PHIIAOA, PREFACE. In the preparation of this book the author has endeavored to keep in mind two guiding principles: first, the importance of simplicity and lucidity in the presentation of facts and theories; and, second, the need of a judicious limitation of the material selected. In regard to the second point every specialist is aware of the bewildering number of researches that have been and are being published in physiology and the closely related sciences, and the difficulty of justly estimating the value of conflicting results. He who seeks for the truth in any matter under discussion is often- times forced to be satisfied with a suspension of judgment, and the writer who attempts to formulate our present knowledge upon almost any part of the subject is in many instances obliged to present the literature as it exists and let the reader make his own deductions. This latter method is doubtless the most satis- factory and the most suitable for large treatises prepared for the use of the specialist or advanced student, but for beginners it is absolutely necessary to follow a different plan. The amount of material and the discussion of details of controversies must be brought within reasonable limits. The author must assume the responsibility of sifting the evidence and emphasizing those con- clusions that seem to be most justified by experiment and obser- vation. As far as material is concerned, it is evident that the selection of what to give and what to omit is a matter of judg-. ment and experience upon the part of the writer, but the present author is convinced that the necessary reduction in material should be made by a process of elimination rather than by con- densation. The latter method is suitable for the specialist with his background of knowledge and experience, but it is entirely unfitted for the elementary student. For the purposes of the latter brief, comprehensive statements are oftentimes misleading, or fail at least to make a Aear impression.. Those subjects that are presented to him must be given with a certain degree of full- ness if he is expected to obtain a serviceable conception of the facts, and it follows that a treatment of the wide subject of physi- ology is possible, when undertaken with this intention, only by the adoption of a system of selection and elimination. The fundamental facts of physiology, its principles and modes 3 197 4 PREFACE. of reasoning are not difficult to understand. The obstacle that is most frequently encountered by the student lies in the com- plexity of the subject, — the large number of more or less dis- connected facts and theories which must be considered in a dis- cussion of the structure, physics, and chemistry of such an intri- cate organism as the human body. But once a selection has been made of those facts and principles which it is most desirable that the student should know, there is no intrinsic difficulty to prevent them from being stated so clearly that they may be comprehended by anyone who possesses an elementary knowledge of anatomy, physics, and chemistry. It is doubtless the art of presentation that makes a text-book successful or unsuccessful. It must be admitted, however, that certain parts of physiology, at this par- ticular period in its development, offer peculiar difficulties to the writers of text-books. During recent years chemical work in the fields of digestion and nutrition has been very full, and as a result theories hitherto generally accepted have been subjected to crit- icism and alteration, particularly as the important advances in theoretical chemistry and physics have greatly modified the attitude and point of view of the investigators in physiology. Some former views have been unsettled and much information has been collected which at present it is difficult to formulate and apply to the explanation of the normal processes of the animal body. It would seem that in some of the fundamental problems of metabolism physiological investigation has pushed its experi- mental results to a point at which, for further progress, a deeper knowledge of the chemistry of the body is especially needed. Cer- tainly the amount of work of a chemical character that bears di- rectly or indirectly on the problems of physiolog}' has shown a re- markable increase within the last decade. Amid the conflicting results of this literature it is difficult or impossible to follow always the true trend of development. The best that the text-book can hope to accomplish in such cases is to give as clear a picture as possible of the tendencies of the time. Some critics have contended that only those facts or conclu- sions about which there is no difference of opinion should be pre- sented to medical students. Those who are acquainted with the subject, however, understand that books written from this standpoint contain much that represents the uncertain compromises of past generations, and that the need of revision is felt as fre- quently for such books as for those constructed on more liberal principles. There does not seem to be any sound reason why a text-book for medical students should aim to present only those conclusions that have crystallized out of the controversies of other times, and ignore entirely the live issues of the day which are PREFACE. 5 of so much interest and importance not only to physiology, but to all branches of medicine. With this idea in mind the author has endeavored to make the student realize that physiology is a growing subject, continually widening its knowledge and read- justing its theories. It is important that the student should grasp this conception, because, in the first place, it is true; and, in the second place, it may save him later from disappointment and distrust in science if he recognizes that many of our conclu- sions are not the final truth, but provisional only, representing the best that can be done with the knowledge at our command. To emphasize this fact as well as to add somewhat to the interest of the reader short historical resumes have been introduced from time to time, although the question of space alone, not to men- tion other considerations, has prevented any extensive use of such material. It is a feature, however, that a teacher might develop with profit. Some knowledge of the gradual evolution of our present beliefs is useful in demonstrating the enduring value of experimental work as compared with mere theorizing, and also in engendering a certain appreciation and respect for knowledge that has been gained so slowly by the exertions of successive generations of able investigators. A word may be said regarding the references to literature inserted in the book. It is perfectly obvious that a complete or approximately complete bibliography is neither appropriate nor useful, however agreeable it may be to give every worker full recognition of the results of his labors. But for the sake of those who may for any reason wish to follow any particular subject more in detail some references have been given, and these have been selected usually with the idea of citing those works which themselves contain a more or less extensive discussion and litera- ture. Occasionally also references have been made to works of historical importance or to separate papers that contain the experi- mental evidence for some special view. TABLE OF CONTENTS. SECTION I. THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. PAGE Chapter I. — The Phenomenon of Contraction 17 The Histological Structure of the Muscle Fiber, 18. — Its Appearance by Polarized Light, 19. — The Extensibility and Elasticity of Muscular Tissue, 19. — The Independent Irritability of Muscle, 22. — Definition and Enumera- tion of Artificial Stimuli, 23. — The Duration of the Simple Muscle Contrac- tion, 25. — The Curve of a Simple Muscle Contraction, 25. — The Latent Pe- riod, 26. — The Phases of Shortening and Relaxation, 26. — Isotonic and Iso- metric Contractions, 27. — Maximal and Submaximal Contractions, 27. — Effect of Temperature upon the Simple Contraction, 28. — Effect of Veratrin on the Simple Contraction, 30. — Contracture, 32. — Fatigue, the Treppe, and Effect of Rapidly Repeated Stimulation, 33. — The Wave of Contraction and Means of Measuring, 33 — Idiomuscular Contractions, 34. — The Energy Liber- ated During a Muscular Contraction, 34. — The Proportional Amount of this Energy Utilized in Work, 35. — The Curve of Work and the Absolute Power of a Muscle, 36.— Definition of Tetanus or Compound Contraction, 39. — The Summation of Contractions, 39. — Discontinuity of the Processes of Contraction in Tetanus, 41. — The Muscle-tone, 41. — The Rate of Stimulation Necessary for Complete Tetanus, 42. — The Tetanic Nature of Voluntary Contractions, 43. — The Ergograph, 45. — Results of Ergographic Experiments, 47. — Sense of Fatigue, 47. — Muscle Tonus, 47. — Rigor Mortis and Rigor Caloris, 49. — The Occurrence and Structure of Plain Muscle Tissue, 52. — Distinctive Prop- erties of Plain Muscle, 52. — The General Properties of Cardiac Muscular Tis- sue, 54. — The Contractility of Cilia and Their General Properties, 54. Chapter II. — The Chemical Composition of Muscle and the Chemi- cal Changes of Contraction and of Rigor Mortis 57 The Composition of Muscle Plasma, 57. — The Proteids of Muscle, 58. — The Carbohydrates of Muscle, 58. — Phosphocarnic Acid, 60. — Lactic Acid in Muscle, 60. — The Nitrogenous Extractives of Muscle, 61. — Pigments of Mus- cle, 61. — Enzymes of Muscle, 61. — Inorganic Constituents of Muscle, 62. — The Chemical Changes in Muscle during Contraction, 62. — The Chemical Changes during Rigor Mortis, 66. — The Relation of the Waste Products to Fatigue, the Chemical Theory of Fatigue, 66. — Theories of the Mechanism of the Contraction of Muscle, 68. Chapter III. — The Phenomenon of Conduction. Properties of the Nerve Fiber 72 General Statement Regarding Property of Conductivity, 72. — Structure of the Nerve Fibre, 73.— Function of the Myelin Sheath, 73.— The Nerve Trunk an Anatomical Unit Only, 74. — Definition of Afferent and Efferent Nerve Fibers, 75. — Classification of Nerve Fibers, 75. — The Bell-Magendie Law of the Composition of the Anterior and the Posterior Roots of the Spinal Nerves, 77. — Cells of Origin of the Anterior and Posterior Root Fibers, 78. — Origin , of the Afferent and Efferent Fibers in the Cranial Nerves, 79. — Independent Irritability of Nerve Fibers, Artificial Nerve Stimuli, 80. — Du Bois-Reymond's Law of Stimulation by the Galvanic Current, 82. — Electrotonus, 83. — Pfliiger's Law of Stimulation, 84. — The Opening and the Closing Tetanus, 86. — Mode of Stimulating Nerves in Man, 86. — Motor Points of Muscles, 88. — Physical and Physiological Poles, 89. Chapter IV. — The Electrical Phenomena Shown by Nerve and Muscle 91 The Demarcation Current, 91. — Construction of the Galvanometer, 92. — Con- struction of the Capillary Electrometer, 94. — Non-polarizable Electrodes, 95. — Action Current or Negative Variation, 96. — Monophasic and Diphasic Action Currents, 98. — The Rheoscopic Frog Preparation, 99. — Relation of Action Current to the Contraction Wave and Nerve Impulse, 100. — The Electrotonic Currents, 101.— The Core-model, 102. » TABLE OF CONTENTS. PAGE Chapter V. — The Nature of the Nerve Impulse and the Nutri- tive Relations of Nerve Fiber and Nerve Cell 104 Historical, 104. — Velocity of the Nerve Impulse, 105. — Relation of the Nerve Impulse to the Wave of Negativity, 107. — Direction of Conduction in the Nerve, 107. — Effect of Various Influences on the Nerve Impulse, 109. — The Fatigue of Nerve Fibers, 110. — The Metabolism of -the Nerve Fiber during Functional Activity, 112. — Theories of the Nerve Impulse, 113. — Qualitative Differences in Nerve Impulses, 115. — Doctrine of Specific Nerve Energies, 116. — Nutritive Relations of Nerve Fibers and Nerve Cells, 116. — Nerve De- generation and Regeneration, 118. — Degenerative Changes in the Central End of the Neuron, 120. SECTION II. THE PHYSIOLOGY OF THE CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. Chapter VI. — Structure and General Properties of the Nerve Cell 122 The Neuron Doctrine, 122. — The Varieties of Neurons, 124. — Internal Struc- ture of the Nerve Cell, 126.— General Physiology of the Nerve Cell, 128. — Summation of Stimuli in Nerve Cells, 130. — Response of the Nerve Cell to Varying Rates of Stimulation, 131. — The Refractory Period of the Nerve Cell, 132. Chapter VII. — Reflex Actions 133 Definition and Historical, 133.— The Reflex Arc, 133.— The Reflex Frog, 135.— Spinal Reflex Movements, 135. — Theory of Co-ordinated Reflexes, 137. — Spinal Reflexes in Mammals, 138. — Dependence of Co-ordinated Reflexes upon the Excitation of the Sensory Endings, 138. — Reflex Time, 139. — Inhibition of Reflexes, 139. — Influence of the Condition of the Cord on its Reflex Activities, 141. — Reflexes from Other Parts of the Nervous System, 142. — Reflexes Through Peripheral Ganglia, Axon Reflexes, 142. — The Tonic Activity of the Spinal Cord, 144. — Effects of Removal of the Spinal Cord, 144. — Knee-jerk, 146. — Reinforcement of the Knee-jerk, 146. — Is the Knee-jerk a Reflex Act? 148. — Conditions Influencing the Extent of the Knee-jerk, 149. — The Knee- jerk and Spinal Reflexes as Diagnostic Signs, 150. — Location of the Centers for the Different Spinal Reflexes, 151. Chapter VIII. — The Spinal Cord as a Path of Conduction 154 Arrangement and Classification of the Nerve Cells in the Cord, 154. — Gen- eral Relations of the Gray and White Matter in the Cord, 156. — The Methods of Determining the Tracts of the Cord, 156. — General Classification of the Tracts of the Cord, 157.— The Names and Locations of the Long Tracts, 159. — The Termination in the Cord of the Fibers of the Posterior Root, 160. — Ascending or Afferent Paths in the Posterior Columns, 161. — Ascending or Afferent Paths in the Lateral Columns, 164. — The Spinal Paths for the Cutane- ous Senses (Touch, Pain, Temperature), 165. — The Homolateral or Contra- lateral Conduction of the Cutaneous Impulses, 166. — The Descending or Ef- ferent Paths in the Anterolateral Columns (Pyramidal System), 167. — Less Well-known Tracts in the Cord, 169. Chapter IX. — The General Physiology of the Cerebrum and Its Motor Functions 171 The Histology of the Cortex, 172. — The Classification of the Systems of Fibers in the Cerebrum (Projection, Association, and Commissural), 173. — Physio- logical Deductions from the Histology of the Cortex, 175. — Extirpation of the Cerebrum, 178. — Localization of Functions in the Cerebrum, Historical, 179. — The Motor Areas of the Cortex, 183. — Differences in Paralysis from Injury to the Spinal Neuron and the Pyramidal Neuron, 185. — Voluntary Motor Paths Other than the Pyramidal Tract, 185.— The Crossed Control of the Muscles and Bilateral Motor Representation in the Cortex, 186. — Are the Motor Areas Exclusively Motor? 186. Chapter X. — The Sense Areas and the Association Areas in the Cortex 188 The Body-sense Area, 188.— The Course of the Fillet, 190.— The Center for Vision, 192. — Histological Evidence of the Course of the Optic Fibers, 193. — The Decussation in the Chiasma, 195. — The Projection of the Retina on the Occipital Cortex, 195. — The Function of the Lower Visual Centers, 197. — The Auditory Center, 198.— Course of the Cochlear Nerve, 199.— The Physio- logical Significance of the Lower Auditory Centers, 201. — Motor Responses from the Auditory Cortex, 202. — The Olfactory Center, 202.— The Olfactory Bulb and its Connections. 202.— The Cortical Center for Smell, 204.— The Cortical Center for Taste, 204.— Aphasia, 204.— Sensory Aphasia, 206.— The Association Areas, 207. — Subdivision of the Association Areas, 209. — The Development of the Cortica Areas, 210. — Physiology of the Corpus Callosum, 213. — Physiology of the Corpora Striata and Optic Thalami, 214. TABLE OF CONTENTS. 9 PAGE Chapter XI. — The Functions of the Cerebellum, the Pons, and the Medulla . 216 Anatomical Structure and Relations of the Cerebellum, 216. — General State- ment of Theories Regarding the Cerebellum, 219. — Experiments upon Abla- tion of the Cerebellum, 220. — Interpretation of the Experimental and Clinical Results, 221. — Conclusions as to the General Functions of the Cerebellum, 223. — The Psychical Functions of the Cerebellum, 224. — Localization of Function in the Cerebellum, 224. — The Functions of the Medulla Oblongata, 225. — The Nuclei of Origin and the Functions of the Cranial Nerves, 226. Chapter XII. — The Sympathetic or Autonomic Nervous System . . 231 General Statements, 231. — Autonomic Nervous System, 232. — The Use of the Nicotin Method, 233. — General Course of the Autonomic Fibers Arising from the Cord, 233. — General Course of the Fibers Arising from the Brain, 234. — General Course of the Fibers Arising from the Sacral Cord, 236. — Normal Mode of Stimulation of Autonomic Nerve Fibers, 236. Chapter XIII. — The Physiology of Sleep 238 General Statements, 238. — Physiological Relations during Sleep, 238. — The Intensity of Sleep, 239. — Changes in the Circulation during Sleep, 242. — • Effect of Sensory Stimulation, 243. — Theories of Sleep, 244. — Hypnotic Sleep. 248. SECTION III. THE SPECIAL SENSES. Chapter XIV. — Classification of the Senses and General State- ments 249 Classification of the Senses, 249. — The Doctrine of Specific Nerve Energies, 251. — The Weber-Fechner Psychophysical Law, 253. Chapter XV. — Cutaneous and Internal Sensations 256 General Statements, 256. — The Punctiform Distribution of the Cutaneous Senses, 256. — Specific Nerve Energies of the Cutaneous Nerves, 258. — The Temperature Senses, 259. — The Sense of Pressure, 260. — The Threshold Stimu- lus and the Localizing Power, 260. — The Pain Sense, 262. — Localization or Projection of Pain Sensations, 263. — Reflected or Misreferred Pains, 263. — The Muscle Sense, 264. — The Quality of the Muscle Sense, 266. — Sensations of Hunger and Thirst, 267.— The Sense of Thirst, 268. Chapter XVI. — Sensations of Taste and Smell 270 The Nerves of Taste, 270.— The End-organ of the Taste Fibers, 272.— Classi- fication of Taste Sensations, 272. — Distribution and Specific Energy of the Fundamental Taste Sensations, 273.— Method of Sapid Stimulation, 275. — The Threshold Stimulus for Taste, 275.— The Olfactory Organ, 275.— The Mechanism of Smelling, 276.— Nature of the Olfactory Stimulus, 277. — The Qualities of the Olfactory Sensations, 277. — Fatigue of the Olfactory Apparatus, 279.— Delicacy of the Olfactory Sense, 280.— Conflict of Olfactory Sensations, 281. — Olfactory Associations, 281. Chapter XVII. — The Eye as an Optical Instrument. Dioptrics of the Eye 282 Formation of an Image by a Biconvex Lens, 282. — Formation of an Image in the Eye, 285.— The Inversion of the Image on the Retina, 287. — The Size of the Retinal Image, 288. — Accommodation of the Eye, 289. — Limit of the Power of Accommodation and Near Point of Distinct Vision, 292. — Far Point of Distinct Vision, 292. — The Refractive Power of the Surfaces in the Eye, 293.— Optical Defects of the Normal Eye, 293.— Spherical Aberration, 294. — Abnormalities in the Refraction of the Eye, Myopia, 295. — Hyperme- tropia, 296. — Presbyopia, 297. — Astigmatism, 297. — Innervation and Control of the Ciliary Muscle and the Muscles of the Iris, 299.— The Accommodation Reflex and the Light Reflex, 301. — Action of Drugs upon the Iris, 303. — The Antagonism of the Sphincter and Dilator Muscles oft the Iris, 304. — The Ophthalmoscope, 305. Chapter XVIII. — The Properties of the Retina. Visual Stimuli and Visual Sensations 308 The Portion of the Retina Stimulated by Light, 308.— The Action Current Caused by Stimulation of the Retina, 309.— The Visual Purple, Rhodopsin, 310.— Extent of the Visual Field, Perimetry, 312.— Central and Peripheral Fields of Vision, 313. — Visual Acuitv, 315.— Relation Between Stimulus and Sensation, Threshold Stimulus, 317.— The Light Adapted and the Dark Adapted 10 TABLE OF CONTENTS. Eye, 318.— Luminosity or Brightness, 319. — Qualities of Visual Sensations, 320.— The Achromatic Series, 320.— The Chromatic Series, 321.— Color Satu- ration and Color Fusion, 321. — The Fundamental Colors, 322. — The Com- plementary Colors, 323. — After Images, Positive and Negative, 323. — Color Contrasts, 324.— Color Blindness, 325.— Dichromatic Vision, 326.— Tests for Color Blindness, 327. — Monochromatic Vision, 327. — Distribution of Color Sense in the Retina, 328.— Functions of the Rods and Cones, 330.— Theories of Color Vision, 331. — Entoptic Phenomena, 336. — Shadows of Corpuscles and Blood-vessels, 336. — Shadows from Lens and Vitreous Humor, 337. Chapter XIX. — Binocular Vision 338 Movements of the Eyeballs, 338. — Co-ordination of the Eye Muscles, Muscular Insufficiency and Strabismus, 339. — The Binocular Field of Vision, 340. — Corresponding or Identical Points, 340. — Physiological Diplopia, 342. — The Horopter, 343. — Suppression of Visual Images, 343.— Struggle of the Visual Fields, 344. — Judgments of Solidity, 344. — Monocular Perspective, 345. — Binocular Perspective, 346. — Stereoscopic Vision, 347. — Explanation of Binoc- ular Perspective, 349. — Judgments of Distance and Size, 349. — Optical De- ceptions, 350. Chapter XX. — The Ear as an Organ for Sound Sensations 353 The Pinna or Auricle, 354. — The Tympanic Membrane, 354.— The Ear Bones, 355. — Mode of Action of the Ear Bones, 356. — Muscles of the Middle Ear, 358. — The Eustachian Tube, 359. — Projection of the Auditory Sensations, 360. — Sensory Epithelium of the Cochlea, 360. — Nature and Action of the Sound Waves, 361. — Classification and Properties of Musical Sounds, 362. — Upper Harmonics or Overtones, 364. — Sympathetic Vibrations and Reso- nance, 366. — Functions of the Cochlea, 366. — Sensations of Harmony and Dis- cord, 369. — Limits of Hearing, 370. Chapter XXI. — Functions of the Semicircular Canals and the Vestibule 372 Position and Structure of the Semicircular Canals, 372. — Flourens's Experi- ments upon the Semicircular Canals, 372. — Temporary and Permanent Effects of the Operations, 374. — Effect of Direct Stimulation of the Canals, 374. — Effect of Section of the Ampullary or the Acoustic Nerve, 375. — Is the Effect of Section of the Canals Due to Stimulation? 375. — Theories of the Functions of the Semicircular Canals, 375. — Summary of the Views upon the Function of the Semicircular Canals, 378. — Functions of the Utriculus and Sacculus, 379. SECTION IV. BLOOD AND LYMPH. Chapter XXII. — General Properties of Blood. Physiology of the Corpuscles 381 Histological Structure of Blood, 381. — Reaction of the Blood, 382. — Specific Gravity of the Blood, 383. — The Red Corpuscles, 384. — Condition of the Hemoglobin in the Corpuscles, 385. — Hemolysis, 385. — Hemolysis Due to Variations in Osmotic Pressure, 386. — Hemolysis Due to Action of Hemoly- sins, 387. — Nature and Amount of Hemoglobin, 389. — Compounds of Hemo- globin with Oxygen and Other Gases, 391. — The Iron in the Hemoglobin, 392. — Crystals of Hemoglobin, 393.— Absorption Spectra of Hemoglobin and Oxyhemoglobin, 394. — Derivative Compounds of Hemoglobin, 398. — Origin and Fate of the Red Corpuscles, 401. — Variations in the Number of Red Cor- Euscles, 402. — Physiology of the Blood Leucocytes, 404. — Variations in Num- er of the Leucocytes, 405. — Functions of the Leucocytes, 405. — Physiology of the Blood Plates, 406. Chapter XXIII. — Chemical Composition of the Blood Plasma; Coagulation; Quantity of Blood; Regeneration After Hemorrhage 408 Composition of the Plasma and Corpuscles. 408. — Proteids of the Blood Plasma, 410. — Serum Albumin, 410. — Paraglobulin (Serum Globulin), 411. — Fibrinogen, 412. — Less Well-known Proteids of the Blood, 413. — Coagula- tion of Blood, 414.— Theories of Clotting, 415.— Why Blood Does Not Clot Within the Vessels, 419. — Intravascular Clotting, 421. — Means of Hastening or of Retarding Coagulation, 421. — Total Quantity of Blood in the Body, 423. — Regeneration of the Blood after Hemorrhage, 424. — Blood Transfu- sion, 426. Chapter XXIV. — Composition and Formation of Lymph 427 General Statements, 427. — Formation of Lymph, 428. — Lymphagogues of the First Class, 430. — Lymphagogues of the Second Class, 431. — Summary of the Factors Controlling the Flow of Lymph, 432. TABLE OF CONTENTS. 11 SECTION V. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE ORGANS OF CIRCULATION OF THE BLOOD AND LYMPH. PAGE Chapter XXV. — The Velocity and Pressure of the Blood Flow. . 434 The Circulation as Seen Under the Microscope, 434. — The Velocity of the Blood Flow, 435. — Mean Velocity in the Arteries, Veins, and Capillaries, 438. — Cause of the Variations in Velocity, 440. — Variations of Velocity with the Heart Beat or Changes in the Blood-vessels, 440. — Time Necessary for a Com- plete Circulation of the Blood, 441. — The Pressure Relations in the Vascular System, 442. — Methods of Recording Blood-pressure, 442. — Systolic, Dias- tolic, and Mean Arterial Pressure, 446. — Method of Measuring Systolic and Diastolic Pressure in Animals, 447. — Data as to the Mean Pressure in Arteries, Veins, and Capillaries, 450. — Methods of Determining Blood -pressure in the Large Arteries of Man, 453. — Normal Pressure in Man and Its Variations, 458. Chapter XXVI. — The Physical Factors Concerned in the Pro- duction of Blood-pressure and Blood- velocity 460 Side Pressure and Velocity Pressure, 460. — The Factors Concerned in Pro- ducing Normal Pressure and Velocity, 463. — General Conditions Influencing Blood-pressure and Blood-velocity, 464. — The Hydrostatic Effect, 465. — Ac- cessory Factors Aiding the Circulation, 466. — The Conditions of Pressure and Velocity in the Pulmonary Circulation, 467. — Variations of Pressure in the Pulmonary Circuit, 468. Chapter XXVIL— The Pulse 469 General Statement, 469. — Velocity of the Pulse Wave, 470. — Form of the Pulse Wave, Sphygmography, 472. — Explanation of the Catacrotic Waves, 474. — Anacrotic Waves, 475. — The Kinds of Pulse in Health and Disease, 475. — Venous Pulse, 475. Chapter XXVIIL— The Heart Beat 477 General Statement, 477.— Musculature of the Auricles and Ventricles, 478. — Contraction Wave of the Heart, 479. — The Electrical Variation, 480. — Change of Form during Systole, 481. — The Apex Beat, 482.— Cardiogram, 482. — In- traventricular Pressure during Systole, 484. — The Heart Sounds, 485. — Events Occurring during a Cardiac Cycle, 488. — Time Relations of Systole and Dias- tole, 489. — Normal Capacity of Ventricle and Work Done by the Heart, 489. — Coronary Circulation during the Heart-beat, 491. — Suction-pump Action of the Heart, 492. — Occlusion of the Coronary Vessels, 494. — Fibrillar Contrac- tions of Heart Muscle, 495. Chapter XXIX. — The Cause and the Sequence of the Heart Beat. Properties of the Heart Muscle 496 General Statement, 496. — The Neurogenic Theory of the Heart Beat, 497. — Myogenic Theory, 498. — Automaticity of the Heart, 500. — Action of Calcium, Potassium, and Sodium Ions on the Heart, 501. — Connection of Inorganic Salts with the Causation of the Beat, 503. — Maximal Contractions of the Heart, 504. — Refractory Period of the Heart Beat, 504. — The Compensatory Pause, 506.— Normal Sequence of the Heart Beat, 507. — Tonicity of the Heart Muscle, 510. Chapter XXX. — The Cardiac Nerves and Their Physiological Action 512 Course of the Cardiac Nerves, 512. — Action of the Inhibitory Fibers, 512. — Analysis of the Inhibitory Action, 514. — Effect of Vagus on the Auricle and the Ventricle, 516. — Escape from Inhibition, 517. — Reflex Inhibition of the Heart-beat, the Cardio-inhibitory Center, 517. — The Tonic Activity of the Cardio-inhibitory Center, 518. — The Action of Drugs on the Inhibitory Ap- paratus, 520. — The Nature of Inhibition, 520. — Course of the Accelerator Fibers, 522. — Action of the Accelerator Fibers, 523. — Tonicity of the Accel- erators and Reflex Acceleration, 524. — The Accelerator Center, 525. Chapter XXXI. — The Rate of the Heart Beat and Its Variations Under Normal Conditions 526 Variations in Rate with Sex, Size, and Age, 526. — Variations Through the Extrinsic Cardiac Nerves, 527. — Variations with Blood-pressure, 527. — With Muscular Exercise, 528.— With the Gases of the Blood, 529. — With Tempera- ture of the Blood, 529. 12 TABLE OF CONTENTS. PAGE) Chapter XXXII. — The Vasomotor Nerves and Their Physiologi- cal Activity 531 Historical, 531. — Methods Used to Determine Vasomotor Action, 532.— The Plethysmograph, 533. — General Distribution and Course of the Vasocon- . strictor Nerve Fibers, 535. — Tonic Activity of the Vasoconstrictors, 538. — The Vasoconstrictor Center, 539. — Vasoconstrictor Reflexes, Pressor and Depressor Fibers, 540. — Depressor Nerve of the Heart, 543. — Vasoconstrictor Centers in the Spinal Cord, 543. — Rhythmical Activity of the Vasocon- strictor Center, 544. — Course and Distribution of the Dilator Fibers, 545. — General Properties of Vasodilator Fibers, 545.— Vasodilator Center and Reflexes, 546. — Vasodilatation Due to Antidromic Impulses, 548. Chapter XXXIII. — The Vasomotor Supply of the Different Organs 550 Vasomotors of the Heart, 550. — Vasomotors of the Pulmonary Arteries, 551. — Circulation in the Brain and Its Regulation, 552. — Arterial Supply, 552. — Venous Supply, 553. — The Meningeal Spaces, 554. — Intracranial Pres- sure, 556. — Effect of Changes in Arterial Pressure upon the Blood -flow Through the Brain, 557. — The Regulation of the Brain Circulation, 559. — Vasomotor Nerves of the Head Region, 561. — Of the Trunk and the Limbs, 562. — Of the Abdominal Organs, 562.— Of the Genital Organs, 562.— Of the Skeletal Muscles, 563. — The Vasomotor Nerves to the Veins, 564. — The Circulation of the Lymph, 564. SECTION VI. PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. Chapter XXXIV. — Historical Statement. The Organs of Ex- ternal Respiration and the Respiratory Movements. . 566 Historical, 566. — Anatomy of Organs of Respiration, 570. — Thorax as a Closed Cavity, 571.— Normal Position of the Thorax, 571. — Inspiration by Contraction of the Diaphragm, 572. — Inspiration by Elevation of the Ribs, 573. — The Muscles of Inspiration, 574. — Muscles of Expiration, 574. — Quiet and Forced Respiratory Movements, Eupnea and Dyspnea, 575. — Costal and Abdominal Types of Respiration, 576. — -Accessory Respiratory Movements, 577. — Registration of the Respiratory Movements, 577. — Volumes of Air Respired, Vital Capacity, Tidal Air, Complemental Air, Supplemental Air, Residual Air, Minimal Air, 579. — Size of the Bronchial Tree, 581. — Arti- ficial Respiration, 581. Chapter XXXV. — The Pressure Conditions in the Lungs and Thorax and Their Influence Upon the Circulation. . . 583 The Intrapulmonic Pressure and Its Variations, 583. — Intrathoracic Pressure, 584. — Variations of, with Forced and Unusual Respirations, 585. — Origin of the Negative Pressure in the Thorax, 586. — Pneumothorax, 587. — Respiratory Action of the Thorax, 587. — Respiratory Waves of Blood-pressure, 588. Chapter XXXVI. — The Chemical and Physical Changes in the Air and the Blood Caused by Respiration 592 The Inspired and Expired Air, 592. — Physical Changes in the Expired Air, 592. — Injurious Action of Expired Air, 593. — Ventilation, 595. — The Gases of the Blood, 596. — The Pressure of Gases, 599. — Absorption of Gases in Liquids, 599.— The Tension of Gases in Solution, 601.— The Condition of Nitrogen in the Blood, 602. — Condition of Oxygen in the Blood, 603. — Con- dition of Carbon Dioxid in the Blood, 604. — The Physical Theory of Respira- tion, 606. — Gaseous Exchanges in the Lungs, 606. — Exchange of Gases in the Tissues, 608. — Secretory Activity of Lungs, 608. Chapter XXXVII. — Innervation of the Respiratory Movements. 610 The Respiratory Center, 610. — Spinal Respiratory Centers, 611. — Automatic Activity of the Respiratory Center, 612. — Reflex Stimulation of the Center, 612. — Afferent Relations of the Vagus to the Center, 614. — The Inspiratory and Inhibitory Fibers of the Vagus, 616. — Respiratory Reflexes from the Larynx, Pharynx, and Nose, 617. — Voluntary Control of the Respiratory Movements, 618. — Nature of the Respiratory Center, 618. — Respiratory Cen- ters in the Midbrain, 619. — Automatic Stimulus to the Respiratory Center, 619. — Cause of the First Respiratory Movements, 621. — Dyspnea, Hyperpnea and Apnea, 622. — Innervation of the Bronchial Musculature, 625. Chapter XXXVIII. — The Influence of Various Conditions Upon the Respiration 627 Effect of Muscular Work on the Respiratory Movements, 627. — Effect of Variations in the Composition of the Air, 627. — High and Low Barometric Pressures, Mountain Sickness, Caisson Disease, 629. — The Respiratory Quo- tient and Its Variations, 631. — Modified Respiratory Movements, 632. TABLE OF CONTENTS. 13 SECTION VII. PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. PAGE Chapter XXXIX. — Movements of the Alimentary Canal 634 Mastication, 634. — Deglutition, 634. — Nervous Control of Deglutition, 638. — Anatomy of the Stomach, 639.— Musculature of the Stomach, 640. — Move- ments of the Stomach, 640. — Effect of the Nerves on the Movements of the Stomach, 643.— Movements of the Intestines, 644. — Peristaltic and Pendular Movements of the Intestines, 646. — Nervous Control of the Intestinal Move- ments, 647. — Effect of Various Conditions on the Intestinal Movements, 648.— Movements of the Large Intestines, 648. — Defecation, 650. — Vomiting, 651. — Nervous Mechanism of Vomiting, 652. Chapter XL. — General Consideration Upon the Composition of the Food and the Action of Enzymes 654 Foods and Foodstuffs, 654. — Accessory Articles of Diet, 656. — Enzymes, Historical, 657. — Reversible Reactions, 659. — Specificity of Enzymes, 661. — Definition and Classification of Enzymes, 661. — General Properties of En- zymes, 663. — Partial List of Enzymes, 664. — Chemical Composition of the Enzymes, 664. Chapter XLI. — The Salivary Glands and Their Digestive Action. 666 Anatomy of the Salivary Glands, 666. — Histological Structure, 668. — Com- position of the Secretion, 669. — The Secretory Nerves, 670. — Trophic and Secretory Nerve Fibers, 672. — Histological Changes during Activity, 674. — Action of Drugs Upon the Secretory Nerves, 676. — Paralytic Secretion, 677. — Normal Mechanism of Salivary Secretion, 678. — Electrical Changes in Glands, 679. — Digestive Action of Saliva; Ptyalin, 679. — Conditions Influencing the Action of Ptyalin, 681. — Functions of the Saliva, 681. Chapter XLII. — Digestion and Absorption in the Stomach 683 Structure of the Gastric Glands, 683. — Histological Changes during Secretion, 684. — Method of Obtaining the Gastric Secretion and Its Normal Composition, 685.— The Acid of Gastric Juice, 687.— Origin of the HC1, 687.— Secretory Nerves of the Gastric Glands, 688. — Normal Mechanism of the Secretion of the Gastric Juice, 689. — Nature and Properties of Pepsin, 691. — Artificial Gastric Juice, 692. — Pepsin-hydrochloric Digestion, 693. — The Rennin En- zyme, 695. — Digestive Changes in the Stomach, 696. — Absorption in the Stomach, 699. Chapter XLIII. — Digestion and Absorption in the Intestines. . 700 Structure of the Pancreas, 700. — Composition of the Secretion, 701. — Secre- tory Nerve Fibers to the Pancreas, 701. — The Curve of Secretion, 702. — Nor- mal Mechanism of Pancreatic Secretion, 703. — Secretin, 703. — Enterokinase, 704. — Digestive Action of Pancreatic Juice, 705. — Significance of Tryptic Digestion, 707. — Action of the Diastatic Enzyme (Amylopsin), 708. — Action of the Lipolytic Enzyme (Lipase, Steapsin), 709. — The Intestinal Secretion (Succus Entericus), 710. — Absorption in the Small Intestine, 712. — Absorp- tion of Carbohydrates, 713.— Absorption of Fats, 714. — Absorption of Pro- teids, 716. — Digestion and Absorption in the Large Intestine, 717.— Bacterial Action in the Small Intestine, 718. — Bacterial Action in the Large Intestine, 719. — Physiological Importance of Intestinal Putrefaction, 719. — Composi- tion of the Feces, 720. Chapter XLIV. — Physiology of the Liver and Spleen 722 Structure of the Liver, 722. — Composition of Bile, 722. — The Bile Pigments, 724. — The Bile Acids, 725. — Cholesterin, 726.— Lecithin, Fats, and Nucleo- albumins, 727. — Secretion of the Bile, 727. — Ejection of the Bile— Function of the Gall-bladder, 728.— Occlusion of the Bile-ducts, 730.— Physiological Im- portance of Bile, 730. — Occurrence of Glycogen, 731. — Origin of Glycogen, 732. — Function of Glycogen, Glycogenic Theory, 734. — Glycogen in the Muscles and Other Tissues, 736. — Conditions Affecting the Supply of Glyco- gen, 736. — Formation of Urea in the Liver, 737. — Physiology of the Spleen, 738. Chapter XLV. — The Kidney and Skin as Excretory Organs .... 741 Structure of the Kidney, 741. — The Secretion of Urine, 742.— Function of the Glomerulus, 744. — Function of the Convoluted Tubule, 746. — Action of Diuretics, 748.— The Blood-flow Through the Kidneys, 749.— The Composi- tion of Urine, 751. — The Nitrogenous Excreta in the Urine, 752. — Origin and Significance of Urea, 753. — Origin and Significance of the Purin Bodies (Uric Acid, Xanthin, Hypoxanthin), 756. — Origin and Significance of the Creatinin, 758. — Hippuric Acid, 759. — The Conjugated Sulphates and the Sulphur Ex- cretion, 759. —Secretion of the Water and Inorganic Salts, 760. — Micturition, 761. — Contractions of the Bladder, 762. — Nervous Mechanism of Micturition, 765.— Excretory Functions of the Skin, 766. — Composition of Sweat, 766. — Secretory Fibers of Sweat Glands, 767. — Sweat Centers, 769. — Sebaceous Secretion, 769. — Excretion of Carbon Dioxid Through the Skin, 770. 14 TABLE OF CONTENTS. Chapter XL VI. — Secretion of the Ductless Glands — Internal Secretion 771 Internal Secretion of Liver, 771. — Internal Secretion of the Thyroid Tissues, 772. — Extirpation of Thyroids and Parathyroids, 772. — Function of the Para- thyroids, 773. — Theories of General Functions of Thyroid and Parathyroids, 774. — Cyon's View of Function of Thyroid, 775. — Structure and Properties of Adrenal Bodies, 775.— General Function of Adrenals, 777. — Pituitary Body, 777. — Internal Secretion of Testis and Ovary, 778. — Internal Secretion of Pancreas, 780. — Internal Secretion of Kidney, 782. SECTION VIII. NUTRITION AND HEAT PRODUCTION AND REGULATION. Chapter XLVII. — General Methods. History of the Proteid Food 783 General Statement, 783. — Nitrogen Equilibrium. 783. — Carbon Equilibrium and Body Equilibrium, 785. — Balance Experiments, 785. — Respiration Chamber, 785. — Effect of Non-proteid Food on Nitrogen Equilibrium, 786. — Nutritive History of the Proteid Food, 786. — Tissue Proteid and Circulating Proteid, 787. — Amount of Proteid Necessary in Normal Nutrition, Question of Luxus Consumption, 788. — Specific Character of Proteid Metabolism, 790. — Nutritive Value of Albuminoids, 791. Chapter XL VIII. — Nutritive History of Carbohydrates and Fats. 793 The Carbohydrate Supply of the Body, 793. — Fate of the Carbohydrates in the Body, 793. — Functions of the Carbohydrate Food, 794. — Nutritive Value of Fats, 796.— Fate of Fats in the Tissues, 797. — Origin of Body Fat, 797.— Origin of Body Fat from Carbohydrates, 798. — Origin of Body Fat from Food Fat, 798. — Cause of the Formation of Fat, Obesity, 799. — General Functions of Fat, 800. Chapter XLIX. — Nutritive Value of the Inorganic Salts and the Accessory Articles of Diet 801 The Inorganic Salts of the Body, 801. — Effect of Ash-free and Ash-poor Diets, 802. — Special Importance of Sodium Chlorid, Calcium and Iron Salts, 802. — The Condiments, Flavors, and Stimulants, 804. — Physiological Effects of Alcohol, 805. Chapter L. — Effect of Muscular Work and Temperature on Body Metabolism; Heat Energy of Foods; Dietetics. . 809 The Effect of Muscular Work, 809.— Effect of Sleep, 810.— Effect of Variations in Temperature, 811. — Effect of Starvation, 812. — The Potential Energy of Food, 814.— Dietetics, 817. Chapter LI. — The Production of Heat in the Body; Its Measure- ment and Regulation; Body Temperature; Calorimetry; Physiological Oxidations 820 Historical Account of Theories of Animal Heat, 820. — Body Temperature in Man, 821.— Calorimetry, 823.— Heat Regulation, 828.— Regulation of Heat Loss, 823. — Regulation of Heat Production, 829. — Existence of Heat Centers and Heat Nerves, 830. — Theories of Physiological Oxidations, 833. SECTION IX. PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. Chapter LII. — Physiology of the Female Reproductive Organs. . . 839 General Statement, 838. — The Graafian Follicle and the Corpus Luteum, 839. — Menstruation and Puberty, 841. — Structural Changes in the Uterus during Menstruation, 842. — The Phenomenon of Heat in Lower Animals, 842. — The Relation of the Ovaries to Menstruation, 843. — Physiological Significance of Menstruation, 845. — Effect of the Menstrual Cycle on Other Functions, 845. — Passage of the Ovum into the Uterus, 846. — Maturation of the Ovum, 847. — Fertilization of the Ovum, 850. — Implantation of the Ovum, 851. — Nutrition of the Ovum; Physiology of the Placenta, 852.— Changes in the Maternal Organism during Pregnancy, 854. — Parturition, 854. — The Mammary Glands, 855. — Connection between the Uterus and the Mammary Glands, 856. — Composition of Milk, 858. TABLE OF CONTENTS. 15 PAGE Chapter LIII. — Physiology of the Male Reproductive Organs. . 860 Sexual Life of Male, 860. — Properties of the Spermatozoa, 860. — Chemistry of the Spermatozoa, 862. — The Act of Erection, 863. — Reflex Apparatus of Erection and Ejaculation, 864. Chapter LIV. — Heredity; Determination of Sex; Growth and Senescence 866 Definition of Heredity, 866. — Evolution and Epigenesis, 866. — Determination of Sex, 868. — Growth and Senescence, 871. APPENDIX. I. — Proteids and Their Classification 874 Definition and General Structure of Proteids, 874. — Reactions of Proteids, 875. —Classification of Proteids, 877.— The Albumins, 878.— The Globulins, 878 — The Albuminates, 878. — Nucleo-albumins or Phosphoproteids, 878. — Pro- teoses and Peptones, 879. — Protamins and Histons, 879. — The Compound Proteids, 880.— The Albuminoids, 880. II. — Diffusion and Osmosis 881 Diffusion, Dialysis, and Osmosis, 881. — Osmotic Pressure, 881, — Electrolytes, 882,-^Gram-molecular Solutions, 883. — Calculation of Osmotic Pressure in Solutions, 883. — Determination of Osmotic Pressure by the Freezing Point, 884.: — Application to Physiological Processes, 884. — Osmotic Pressure of Proteids, 884. — Isotonic, Hypertonic, and Hypotonic Solutions, 885. — Diffu- sion or Dialysis of Soluble Constituents, 885. — Diffusion of Proteids, 886. Index 887 (?o6> ■ A TEXT-BOOK OF PHYSIOLOGY SECTION I. THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. CHAPTER I. THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. The tissues in the mammalian body in which the property of contractility has been developed to a notable extent are the mus- cular and the ciliated epithelial cells. The functional value of the muscles and the cilia to the body as an organism depends, in fact, upon the special development of this property. The muscular tissues of the body fall into three large groups, considered from either a histological or a functional standpoint, — namely, the striated skeletal muscle, the striated cardiac muscle, and the plain muscle. These tissues exhibit certain marked differences in properties which are described farther on. In each group, moreover, there are certain minor differences in structure which are associated with differences in properties; thus, skeletal muscle from different re- gions of the same animal may show variations in properties, — for instance, in the rapidity of contraction; and similar, perhaps more marked differences are observed in the plain muscular tissue of various organs. The muscular tissues from animals belonging to different classes exhibit naturally even wider variations in proper- ties, and these differences in some cases are not associated with visible variations in structure. 2 17 18 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUCLES AND NERVE. THE PHYSIOLOGY OF SKELETAL MUSCLE TISSUE. This tissue makes up the essential part of the skeletal muscles by- means of which our voluntary movements are effected. Each muscle fiber arises from a single cell and in its fully developed condition may be regarded as a multinuclear giant cell. It is inclosed entirely in a thin, structureless, elastic membrane, the sarcolemma. The material of the fiber is supposed to be semifluid or viscous when in the living condition; it is designated in general as the muscle plasma. There is on record an interesting observation by Kuhne * which seems to demonstrate the fluid nature of the living muscle substance. He hap- pened, on one occasion, to find a frog's muscle fiber containing a nematode worm within the sarcolemma. The animal swam readily from one end of the fiber to the other, pushing aside the cross bands, which fell into place Fig. 1. — A cross-section of muscle fiber of rabbit. The bundles of fibrils are dark; the intervening small amount of sarcoplasm is represented by the clear spaces. — (Kolliker.) ; - V- - ■ .-. Fig. 2. — Cross-section of two muscle fibers of the fly: Ms, The columns of fibrils; Sp, the sar ooplasm. — {Schieffer- decker.) again after the animal had passed. At one end, where the fiber had been injured, the worm was unable to force its way. The muscle substance at this point was dead and apparently had passed into a solid condition. The fact that the cross bands were displaced only temporarily by the movement and fell back into their normal position would indicate that they may have a more solid structure. Disregarding the nuclei, the muscle plasma consists of two different structures : the fibrils, which are long and thread-like and run the length of the fiber, and the intervening sarcoplasm. The fibrils consist of alternating dim and light discs or segments, which, coming together in the different fibrils, give the cross-striation that is characteristic. In mammalian muscles the fibrils are grouped more or less distinctly into bundles or columns, between which lies the scanty sarcoplasm. The relative amount of sarcoplasm to fibrillar substance varies greatly in the striped muscles of different * Kuhne, "Archiv fur pathologische Anatomie," 26, 222, 1863. THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 19 animals, as is indicated in the accompanying illustration. The evidence from comparative physiology indicates that the fibrils are the contractile element of the fiber, while the sarcoplasm, it may be assumed, possesses a general nutritive function. Com- parative histology suggests that, in the fibrils, we possess, so to speak, a mechanism adapted to rapid contraction, and that the perfection of the mechanism — that is, the rapidity of its contraction — is indicated by the clearness of the cross-striation. The fibril, moreover, shows two kinds of sub- stance, the alternating dim and light sub- stance, and these two materials are obviously different in physical structure as seen by ordinary light. When examined by polarized light this difference becomes more evident, for the dim substance possesses the property of double refraction. When the muscle fiber is placed between crossed Nicol prisms the dim bands appear bright, while the light bands remain dark, as is shown in Fig. 3. From this standpoint the material of the light bands in the normal fibrils is spoken of as isotropous, while the dim bands are anisotropous. The anisotropic material of the dim bands is composed of doubly refracting positive uniaxial particles, and Engelmann has shown that such particles may be discovered in all contractile tissues. The inference made by him is that this anisotropic substance is the contractile material in the protoplasm, the machinery, so to speak, through which its shortening is accomplished. In the striated fiber this conclusion is supported by the fact, repre- sented in Fig. 3, that during contraction liquid passes from the isotropous (light) band into the anisotropous (dim) band.* The Extensibility and Elasticity of Muscular Tissue. — The muscular tissue when acted upon by a weight extends quite readily, and when the weight is removed it regains its original form by virtue of its elasticity. In our bodies the muscles stretched from bone to bone are, in fact, in * Biedermann, "Electro-physiology," vol. i, translated by Welby, and Engelmann, "Archiv fur die gesammte Physiologie," 18, 1. b £— d 1 e I fm fl km Fig. 3. — To show the appearance of the dim (anisotropic) and light (isotropic) bands at rest and in contraction, as seen by ordinary and by polar- ized light. The figure rep- resents a muscle fiber (beetle) in which the lower portion has been fixed in a condition of contraction. — (Engelmann.) 20 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. a state of elastic tension. If a muscle is severed by an incision across its belly the ends retract. The extensibility and elasticity of the muscles add to the effectiveness of the muscular-skeletal machinery. A muscle that is in a state of elastic tension con- tracts more promptly and more effectively for a given stimur- lus than one which is entirely relaxed. Moreover, in our joints the arrangement of antagonists — flexors and extensors — is such that the contraction of one moves the bone against the pull of the extensible and elastic antagonist. It would seem that the movements of the skeleton must gain much in smoothness and delicacy by this arrangement. The physical advantages of the extensibility and elasticity of muscular tissue are evident not Fig. 4. — n, Curve of extension of a rubber band, to show the equal extension for equal increments of weight. The band had an initial load of 17 gms., and this was increased by increments of 3 gms. in each of the nine extensions, the final load being 44 gms. The line joining the ends of the ordinates is a straight line. 6, Curve of extension of a frog's muscle (gastrocnemius). The initial load and the increment of weight were the same as with the rubber. The curve shows a decreasing extension for equal increments. The line join- ing the ends of the ordinates is curved. only in the contractions of our voluntary muscles, but, as we shall see, in a striking way also in the circulation, in which the force of the heart beat is stored and economically distributed, as it were, by the elastic tension of the distended arteries. The extensibility of muscular tissue has been studied in comparison with the extensi- bility of dead elastic bodies. With regard to the latter it is known that the strain that the body undergoes is proportional within the limits of elasticity to the stress put upon it. If, for instance, weights are attached to a rubber band suspended at one end the amount of extension of the band will be directly proportional to the weights used. If the extensions are measured the relation- ship may be represented as shown in the accompanying figure, the equal increments in weight being indicated by laying off equal THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 21 distances on the abscissa, and the resulting extensions by the height of the ordinates dropped from each point. If the ends of the ordinates are joined the result is a straight line. When a similar experiment is made with a living muscle it is found that the extension is not proportional to the weight used. The amount of extension is greatest in the beginning and decreases propor- tionately with new increments of weight. If the results of such an experiment are plotted, as above, representing the equal incre- ments of weight by equal distances along the abscissa and the resulting extensions by ordinates dropped from these points, then upon joining the ends of the ordinates we obtain a curve concave to the abscissa. At first the muscle shows a relatively large exten- sion, but the effect becomes less and less with each new increment of weight, the curve at the end approaching slowly to a horizontal. If the weight is in- creased until it is sufficient to overcome the elasticity of the muscle the curve is altered — it becomes convex to the abscissa, or, in other words, the amount of extension increases with increasing increments of weight up to the point of rupture, as is shown in the accom- panying curve* (Fig. 5). Haycraftf calls attention to the fact that under normal conditions the physiological extension of the frog's muscles in the body is equal to that produced by a weight of 10 to 15 gms., and that when the excised muscle is extended by weights below this limit it follows the law of dead elastic bodies, giving equal extensions for equal increments of weight. It is only after passing this limit that the law stated above holds good. It should be added also that the amount of extension exhibited by a muscle or other living tissue placed under a stress varies with the time that the stress is allowed to act. The muscle is composed of viscous material, and yields slowly to the force acting upon it. In experiments of this kind, therefore, the weights should be allowed to act for equal intervals * See Marey, "Du mouvement dans les fonctions de la vie," 1868, p. 284. t Haycraft, "Journal of Physiology," 31, 392, 1904. Fig. 5. — Curve given by Marey to show the effect upon the extension of muscle caused by increasing the load regularly to the point of rupture: o-x. The abscissa gives the normal length of muscle; at a the limit of elasticity is passed and the muscle lengthens by increasing extensions for equal increments; at x rupture (750 gms. for frog's gastrocnemius) . 22 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. of time. It has been shown that the extensibility of a muscle is greater in the contracted than in the resting state. The curve of extension described above for skeletal muscle holds also for so-called plain muscle. This latter tissue forms a portion of the walls of the various viscera, the stomach, bladder, uterus, blood-vessels, etc., and the facts shown by the above curve enter frequently into the explanation of the physical phenomena exhibited by the viscera. For instance, it follows from this curve that the force of the heart beat will cause less expansion in an artery already distended by a high blood-pressure than in one in which the blood-pressure is lower. The Irritability and Contractility of Muscle. — Under normal conditions in the body a muscle is made to contract by a stimulus received from the central nervous system through its motor nerve. If the latter is severed the muscle is paralyzed. We owe to Haller, the great physiologist of the eighteenth century, the proof that a muscle thus isolated can still be made to contract by an artificial stimulus — e. g., an electrical shock — applied directly to it. This significant discovery removed from physiology the old and harmful idea of animal spirits, which were supposed to be generated in the central nervous system and to cause the swelling of a muscle during contraction by flowing to it along the connecting nerve. But to remove a muscle from the body and make it contract by an artificial stimulus does not prove that the muscle substance itself is capable of being acted upon by the stimulus, since in such an experiment the endings of the nerve in the muscle are still intact, and it may be that the stimulus acts only on them and thus affects the muscle indirectly. In a number of ways, however, physiologists have found that the muscle substance can be made to contract by a stimulus applied directly to it, and therefore exhibits what is known as independent irritability. The term irritability, according to modern usage, means that a tissue can be made to exhibit its peculiar form of functional activity when stimulated, — e. g., a muscle cell will contract, a gland cell will secrete, etc., — and inde- pendent irritability in the case under consideration means simply that the muscle gives its reaction of contraction when artificial stimuli are applied directly to its substance. This conception of irritability was first introduced by Francis Glisson (1597-1677), a celebrated English physician.* Subsequent writers frequently used the term as synonymous with contractility and as applicable only to the muscle. But it is now used for all living tissues in the sense here indicated. A simple proof of the independent irritability of a striated muscle is obtained by cutting the motor nerve going to it and stimulating the muscle after several days. * See Foster's " History of Physiology," p. 287. THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 23 We know now that in the course of several days the severed nerve fibers degenerate completely down to their terminations in the muscle fibers, and the muscle, thus freed from its nerve fibers by the process of degeneration, can still be made to contract by an artificial stimulus. The classical proof of the independent irri- tability of muscle fibers was given by Claude Bernard, the great French physiologist of the nineteenth century. He made use of the so-called arrow poison of the South American Indians. This substance or mixture of substances is known generally under the name curare; it is prepared from the juices of several plants (strychnos) (Thorpe). The poisonous part of the material is soluble in water, and Bernard showed that when such an extract is injected into the blood or hypodermically it paralyzes the motor nerves at their peripheral end, so that direct stimulation of these nerves Fig. 6. — The induction coil as used for physiological purposes (du Bois-Reymond pattern): A, The primary coil; B, the secondary coil; P' , binding posts to which are at- tached the wires from the battery, they connect with the ends of coil A; P", binding posts connecting with ends of coil B, through which the induction current is led off; S, the slide, with scale, in which coil B is moved to alter its distance from A. is ineffective. Direct stimulation of the muscle substance, on the contrary, causes a contraction.* Artificial Stimuli. — If we designate the stimulus that the muscle receives normally from its nerve as its normal stimulus, all other forms of energy which may be used to start its contraction may be grouped under the designation artificial stimuli. Experi- ments have shown that a contraction may be aroused by mechanical stimuli, — for instance, by a sharp blow applied to the muscle; by thermal stimuli, — that is, by a sudden change in temperature; by chemical stimuli, — for example, by the action of concentrated solu- tions of salts, and finally by electrical stimuli. In practice, however, only the last form of stimulus is found to 'be convenient. The mechanical and thermal stimuli cannot be well applied without at the same time injuring the muscle substance, and the same is prob- ably true of chemical stimuli, which possess the disadvantage, more- over, of acting separately on the different fibers of which the muscle * " Lecons sur les effets des substances toxiques et medicamenteuses," 1857, pp. 238 et seq. 24 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. is composed. Electrical stimuli, on the contrary, are applied easily, are readily controlled as regards their intensity, and affect all the fibers simultaneously, thus giving a co-ordinated contraction of the entire bundle, as is the case with the normal stimulus. For electrical stimulation we may use the galvanic current taken directly from the battery, or the induced or so-called faradic current ob- tained from an induction coil. Under most conditions the latter is more convenient, since it gives brief shocks, the strength and number of which can be controlled readily. The form in which this instrument is used in experimental work in physiology we owe to du Bois-Reymond (1849-); hence it is frequently known as the du Bois-Reymond induction coil. Experimental physiology a B Fig. 7. — Schema of induction apparatus. — (Lombard.) b represents the galvanic battery connected by wires to the primary coil, A. On the course of one of these wires is a key (k) to make and break the current. B shows the principle of the secondary coil, and the connection of its two ends with the nerve of a nerve-muscle preparation. When the battery current is closed or made in i, a brief current of high intensity is induced in B. This is known as the making or closing shock. When the battery current is broken in A, a second brief induction current is aroused in B. This is known as the breaking or opening shock. owes a great deal to this simple and serviceable instrument. A figure and brief description of the apparatus is appended (Figs. 6 and 7). SIMPLE CONTRACTION OF MUSCLE. Experiments may be made upon the isolated muscles of various animals, but ordinarily in physiological laboratories one of the muscles (gastrocnemius) of the hind leg of the frog is employed. If such a muscle is isolated and connected with the terminals from an induction coil it may be stimulated by a single shock or by a series of rapidly repeated shocks. The contraction that results from a single stimulus is designated as a simple contraction. In the frog's muscle it is very brief, lasting for 0.1 second or less; but in this, as in other respects, cross-striated muscular tissue varies in different animals,* as is shown by the accompanying table, which gives an idea of the range of rapidity of contraction. * Cash, "Archiv f. Anat. u. Physiol./' 1880, suppl. volume, p. 147. THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 25 DURATION OF A SIMPLE MUSCULAR CONTRACTION. Insect 0.003 sec. Man 0.050 " Frog 0.100 " Terrapin 1.000 " The series may be continued by the figures obtained from the plain muscle, thus: The involuntary muscle (mammal) 10.00 Foot muscle of slug* (Ariolimax) 20.00 There is reason to believe that the rapidity of contraction is re- lated to the distinctness of the cross-striation. This is indicated by the properties of the so-called red and pale muscles that occur in some animals — the rabbit, for instance. The pale muscles con- tract much more rapidly than the red ones, and corresponding with Fig. 8. — Curve of simple muscular contraction. this fact it is found that the cross-striation is more distinct in the former. As was explained above, the active agent in contraction is contained in the dim bands of the fibers, and the more highly differentiated this structure becomes the more perfect apparently is its work as a mechanism for shortening. According to Cash, the duration of contraction of the soleus muscle (red) in rabbits is one second^ while that of the gastrocnemius medialis (white) is only 0.25 second. The Curve of Contraction. — When a contracting muscle is attached to a lever this lever may be made to write upon a smoked surface and thus record the movement, more or less magnified according to the leverage chosen. If the recording surface is sta- tionary the record obtained is a straight line and indicates only the extent of the shortening. If, however, the recording surface is in movement during the contraction the record will be in the form of a curve, which, making use of the system of right-angled co-ordinates, * Carlson, "American Journal of Physiology," 10, 418, 1904. 26 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. will indicate not only the true extent of the shortening, but also the amount of shortening or subsequent relaxation at any moment during the entire period. To obtain such records from the rapidly contracting frog's muscle it is evident that the recording surface must move with considerable rapidity and with a uniform velocity. A curve of this kind is represented in Fig. 8, on page 25. C rep- resents the axis of abscissas and gives the factor of time. A vertical ordinate erected at any point on C gives the extent of shortening at that moment. Below the curve of the muscle is the record of the vibrations of a tuning fork giving 50 double vibra- tions per second ; that is, the distance from crest to crest represents an interval of -g^ of a second. Three principal facts are brought out by an analysis of the curve: I. The latent period. By this is meant that the muscle does not begin to shorten until a certain time after the stimulus is applied. On the curve the stimulus enters the muscle at S, and the distance between this point and the beginning of the rise of the curve interpreted in time is the latent period. II. The phase of shortening, which has a definite course and at its end immediately passes into III — the phase of relaxation. The Latent Period. — In the contraction of the isolated frog's muscles as usually recorded the latent period amounts to 0.01 sec, but it is generally assumed that this period is exaggerated by the method of recording used, since the elasticity of the muscle itself prevents the immediate registration of the movement. By improve- ments in methods of technique the latent period for a fresh muscle may be reduced to as little as 0.005 or even 0.004 sec. Under the conditions in the body, however, the muscle contracts against a load, as when lifting a lever; hence, we may assume that normally there is a lost time of at least 0.01 sec. after the stimulus enters the muscle. In addition to the latent period due to the elasticity of the muscle it is probable that a brief amount of time actually elapses after the stimulus enters the muscle before the act of shortening begins ; some time is taken up in the chemical changes and the effect of these changes in putting the mechanism of con- traction into play (see below on the Theory of Muscle Contractions). The latent period varies greatly in muscles of different kinds, and in the same muscle varies with its conditions as regards temperature, fatigue, load to be raised, etc. The Phases of Shortening and of Relaxation. — In the normal frog's muscle the phase of shortening for a simple contraction occu- pies about 0.04 second, while the relaxation is a trifle longer, 0.05 sec. Naturally in muscles whose duration of contraction differs from that of the frog the time values for the shortening and the relaxation exhibit corresponding differences. Many conditions, some of which will be described below, alter the duration of the simple contraction, THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 27 and it is noteworthy that, in general, it is the phase of relaxation which is most readily affected by these changes. Isotonic and Isometric Contractions. — In the method of recording the shortening of the muscle that is described above the muscle is supposed to con- tract against a constant load which it can lift. Such a contraction is spoken of as an isotonic contraction. If the muscle is allowed to contract against a tension too great for it to overcome — a stiff spring, for instance — it is prac- tically prevented from shortening, and a contraction of this kind, in which the length of the muscle remains unchanged, is spoken of as an isometric contraction. A curve of such a contraction may be obtained by magnifying greatly, by means of levers, the slight change in the stiff spring against which the muscle is contracting. Such a curve gives a picture of the liberation of energy within the muscle during contraction. The usual oval form of dynamometer employed to record the grip of the flexors of the fingers gives an isometric record of the energy of contraction of these muscles. Fig. 9. — Effect of varying the strength of stimulus. The figure shows the effect upon the gastrocnemius muscle of a frog of gradually increasing the stimulus (breaking induction shock) until maximum contractions were obtained. The stimuli were then decreased in strength and the contractions fell off through a series of gradually decreasing submaximal contractions. The series up and down is not absolutely regular owing to the difficulty of obtaining a regular increase or decrease in the stimulus. (The prolongations of the curves below the base line are due to the elastic extension of the muscle by the weight dur- ing relaxation.) Effect of Strength of Stimulus upon the Simple Contraction. — The strength of electrical stimuli can be varied conveniently and with great accuracy. When the stimulus is of such a strength as to produce a just visible contraction it is spoken of as a minimal stimulus and the resulting contraction as a minimal contraction. Stimuli of less strength than the minimal are designated as sub- minimal. If one increases gradually the intensity of the electrical current used as a stimulus without altering its duration, beginning with a stimulus sufficient to cause a minimal contraction, the result- ing contractions increase proportionally up to a certain maximum 28 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. beyond which further increase of stimulus, other conditions remain- ing the same, causes no greater extent of shortening. Contrac- tions between the minimal and the maximal are designated as submaximal.* (See Fig. 9.) Effect of Temperature upon the Simple Contraction. — Varia- tions in temperature affect both the extent and the duration of the contraction. The relationship is, however, not a simple one in the case of the frog's muscle upon which it has been studied most fre- quently. If we pay attention to the extent of the contraction alone it will be found that at a certain temperature, 0° C, or slightly below, the muscle loses its irritability entirely. As its temperature is raised a given stimulus, chosen of such a strength as to be maximal for the muscle at room temperatures, causes greater and greater Fig. 10. — Curve showing the effect of temperature. The temperatures at which the contractions were obtained are indicated on the figure. In this experiment a large resis- tance was introduced into the secondary circuit so that changes in the resistance of the muscle itself due to heating could not affect the strength of the stimulus. contractions up to a certain maximum, which is reached at about 5° to 9° C. As the temperature rises beyond this point the con- tractions decrease somewhat to a minimum that is reached at about 15° to 18° C. Beyond this the contractions again increase in extent to a second maximum at about 26° to 30° C, this maxi- mum being in some cases greater, and in others less than the first maximum. Beyond the second maximum the contractions again decrease rather rapidly as the temperature rises until at a certain temperature, 37° C, irritability is entirely lost (Fig. 10). If the tem- perature is raised somewhat beyond this latter point heat rigor makes its appearance, and the muscle may be considered as dead. The re- lationship between temperature and extent of contraction, therefore, * Fick, " Untersuchungen iiber elektrische Nervenreizung," Braun- schweig, 1864. THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 29 may be expressed by a curve such as is represented in Fig. 11, in which there are two maxima and two points at which irritability is lost . The second maximum indicates a fact of general physiological in- terest,— namely, that in all of the tissues of the body there is a certain high temperature at which optimum activity is exhibited, and if the temperature is raised beyond this point functional activity becomes more and more depressed. The point of optimum effect is not identi- cal for the different tissues of the same animal, much less so for those Fig. 11. — Curve to show the effect of a rise of temperature from 0° C. to 38° C. upon the height of contraction of frog's muscle. The first maximum at 9° C, the second at 28° C. Beyond 38° C. the muscle lost its irritability and went into rigor mortis. Zb 6" io' IS' 20' 2S' S ,&■ /a, icu vM- — ^* n Fig. 35. — Two schemata to show the relation between the physical and the physio- logical electrodes or poles. Each schema represents the forearm with the median nerve, M . In / the stimulating electrode is the cathode; the threads of current which have started from the anode (the indifferent electrode) placed elsewhere, converge to this pole. Where these threads enter the nerve we have a series of physiological anodes, a; where they leave, a series of physiological cathodes, c. In // the stimulating electrode is the anode. The threads of current leave this pole to traverse the body toward the indifferent electrode (cathode). Where they enter and leave the nerve we have, as in the first case, physio- logical anodes and cathodes, now, however, on the opposite sides of the nerve. methods of stimulation. The nerve trunk under such circum- stances fails to respond to either form of stimulus, induced or gal- vanic. The muscle, on the other hand, while it fails to respond to induction shocks, is stimulated by the galvanic current and shows certain qualitative changes in its reactions to this latter form of stimulation. For instance, under these conditions the A C C is ob- tained with less current than the C C C, a relation which is just the reverse of the normal. This qualitative and quantitative change in reaction to the galvanic current, and the loss of irritability to the induced current, constitute what is known as the reaction of degen- eration. Distinction between Physical and Physiological Poles. — The facts stated above seem to show, at first sight, that by the unipolar method we may obtain both an opening and a closing 90 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. shock at either the cathode or anode, — a result which is in apparent contradiction to the general law that the making or closing stimulus occurs only at the cathode and the breaking or opening stimulus only at the anode. This apparent contra- diction is readily explained when we remember that in the unipolar method the active electrode rests upon the skin over the nerve, and that the threads of current radiating from this point enter the nerve at one point and leave it at another. Evidently, therefore, so far as the nerve is concerned, there will be an anode where the current is considered as entering the nerve and a cathode where it leaves it, so that under the active electrode, whether this is physically an anode or cathode, there will be, as regards the nerve, a series of what may be called physiological cathodes and anodes. The closing shock arises at these cathodes, the opening shock at the anodes. The position of the series of anodes and cathodes will vary according as the active electrode is an anode or cathode, as is indicated in the accompanying diagram (Fig. 35). CHAPTER IV. THE ELECTRICAL PHENOMENA SHOWN BY NERVE AND MUSCLE. The Demarcation Current. — Our definite knowledge of the electrical properties of living tissue began with the celebrated in- vestigations of du Bois-Reymond* (1843). When a muscle or nerve is removed from the body, and, in the case of the muscle, one tendinous end is cut off, it is found that the cut end has an electrical potential that differs from that of the uninjured longi- tudinal surface of the preparation. Following the usual nomen- clature, the cut end is electronegative as regards the longitudinal surface. If, therefore, the longitudinal surface is connected by a conductor with the cut surface a current will flow from the former to the latter, as is indicated in the accompanying diagram. Fig. 36. — Schema showing the course of the demarcation current in an excised nerve, when a point on the longitudinal and one on the cut surface are united by a conductor. While the direction of the current through the conductor con- necting the two points is from the longitudinal to the cut surface the current may be considered as being completed in the opposite direction within the substance of the muscle or nerve, as shown in the diagram. We may, in fact, consider an excised nerve or muscle as a battery, the cut end representing the zinc plate and the longitudinal surface the copper plate. Within the battery the direction of the current is from zinc to copper, from cut end to longitudinal surface; outside the battery the direction is from copper to zinc, from longitudinal to cut surface. If two wires are connected with the muscle or nerve the end of the one attached to the longitudinal surface will represent the positive pole or anode, the end of the one attached to the cut end will represent the cathode * "Untersuchungen iiber thierische Elektricitat," du Bois-Reymond, 1848-1860. 91 92 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. or negative pole. On joining the ends of the wires a current will pass from positive to negative pole. A current of this character from an excised nerve or muscle is, of course, small in amount and to detect it one must make use of a delicate electrometer of some sort (see below). Du Bois- Reymond considered that the difference in electrical potential which gives rise to this current exists normally in the muscle, although masked by an opposite condition in the tendinous ends, and he therefore spoke of the currents as the natural muscle or natural nerve current. It has since been shown by Hermann that this view is incorrect, that the perfectly normal uninjured muscle or nerve has the same electrical potential throughout and will therefore give no current when any two points are con- nected by a conductor. Moreover, the completely dead muscle or nerve shows no current. The difference in poten- tial that is found in the excised nerve or muscle is due, according to Hermann, to the fact that at the cut end the nerve or muscle is injured. The chemical changes that take place as a result of the injury make the tissue electronegative as regards the un- changed living substance elsewhere. For this reason Hermann described the current obtained as a demarcation current ; others have called it the cur- rent of injury. • 0 Fig. 37. — Schema showing the principle of construction of the galvanometer: M, The mag- net suspended by a thread; B, the battery, with the wires lead- ing off the current encircling the magnet. Means of Demonstrating the Demarcation Current. — The demarca- tion current and other electrical conditions to be described require especial apparatus for their study. To detect the existence of a current physiologists use either a high-resistance galvanometer or a capillary electrometer. The galvanometers employed are usually of two types, the Kelvin reflecting galvanometer or the d'Arsonval form. The principle of the galvanometer lies in the fact that a magnetic needle is deflected when an electrical current passes through a wire in its vicinity. As shown in the accompanying dia- gram, if a magnetic needle is swung by a delicate thread so as to move easily it will come to rest in the magnetic meridian with its north pole pointing north. If now a wire is curved round it and a battery current is sent through this wire the needle will be deflected to the right if the current passes in one direction and to the left if it passes in the opposite direction. The move- ment of the needle is an indication of the presence and direction of the electrical current in the wire. The extent of deflection of the needle may be used to measure the strength of the current by ascertaining the amount of deflection caused by a standard battery. The effect of the current upon the needle increases with the number of turns of wire, so that delicate galvanometers constructed upon this principle are spoken of as high resistance galvanom- ELECTRICAL PHENOMENA. 93 eters, the great length of wire used making, of course, a high resistance. Instead of having the coil through which the current passes kept in a fixed position and the magnet delicately swung or poised, the reverse arrangement may be used, — that is, the coil may be swung between the poles of a fixed magnet. Under these circumstances if a current is sent through the coil this latter will move with reference to the magnet. A galvanometer con- structed on this principle is designated as a d'Arsonval galvanometer, after the physiologist who first employed this arrangement. The d'Arsonval form of galvanometer possesses many practical advantages for physiological work, and it may suffice to give the details of this form alone. In the d'Arsonval form the magnet is fixed while the coil of wire through which the current passes is swung by a very delicate thread of quartz, silk fiber, or phosphor- bronze. The principle of the arrangement is shown in the accompanying diagram (Fig. 39) and the complete instrument in Fig. 38. A large horse- shoe magnet (n, s) is fixed permanently and between the poles is swung a coil (c) of delicate wire, the two ends of the wire being connected with binding Fig. 38. — D'Arsonval galvanometer as modified by Rowland. posts in the frame of the instrument. The coil is held in place below by a delicate spiral. In Fig. 39 it will be seen that the delicate thread suspending the coil carries just above the coil a small mirror, m, and a plate of thin mica or aluminum. The mirror is deflected with the coil, and when viewed through the telescope pictured in Fig. 38 the image of the scale above the telescope is reflected in this mirror. As the coil and mirror are twisted by the action of the current passing through the former the reflection of the scale in the mirror is displaced. By means of a cross hair in the telescope the angle of deflection may be read upon the reflected scale. The aluminum vane back of the mirror makes the system dead-beat so that when a deflection is obtained the system comes quickly to rest with few or no oscillations. If the coil of wire contains sufficient turns, enough to give a total resistance of two to three thousand ohms, and the poles of the magnet are brought very close to the coil, the instrument may be given a delicacy sufficient to study accurately the muscle and nerve currents. In such an instrument the effect of the earth's magnetism may be neglected and the galvanometer may be hung upon any support without reference to the magnetic meridian. 94 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. The Capillary Electrometer. — The movable system of a galvanometer possesses considerable weight, therefore inertia; so that it will not indicate accurately the presence or extent of very brief electrical currents such as have to be studied in physiology in some cases. For purposes of this kind a simple instrument known as the capillary electrometer is employed. The principle of the construction of this instrument is illustrated in Fig. 40. A glass tube, a, is drawn out at one end into a very fine capillary the end of which dips Fig. 39. — Diagram of struc- ture of the d'Arsonval galvanom- eter, c is the coil of fine wire through which the current is passed. It is swung by a fine thread of phosphor-bronze so as to lie between and close to the poles — (n) north pole, and (s) south pole — of the magnet. Just above the magnet the thread car- ries a mica or aluminum vane to which is attached a small mirror. The scale of the instrument is re- flected in this mirror and is observed through the telescope shown in Fig. 38. Fig. 40. — Schema of capillary electrometer arranged to show the demarcation current in muscle (Lombard) : a, The glass tube containing mercury and drawn to a fine capillary below ; c, the receptacle containing mercury by raising which the mercury can be driven into the capil- lary of a; /, a vessel with glass sides containing mercury below, and above dilute sulphuric acid into which the capillary of a dips; E, the micro- cope for observing the mercury thread in the capillary; m, the muscle; g and h, the wires touching the longitudinal and cut surfaces of the muscle. The current flows as indicated by the small arrows; d, the capillary thread of mercury as seen under the microscope. into some diluted sulphuric acid contained in the vessel (/) . At the bottom of this vessel is a layer of mercury connecting with a wire, g, fused into the glass vessel. The tube a is partially filled with redistilled mercury, which penetrates for a short distance into the capillary. By means of pressure ap- plied from above c, the mercury can be forced through the capillary. Then by diminishing the pressure the mercury can be brought back into the capil- lary a certain distance, drawing after it some of the dilute sulphuric acid. The mercury in tube a is connected with the other pole of the battery by a ELECTRICAL PHENOMENA. 95 —3 wire fused into its wall and dipping into the mercury By regulating the pressure on the mercury the point of contact between the thread^ of mercury and the sulphuric acid in the capillary, d, can be brought to any desired posi- tion. An equilibrium is then established which will remain constant as long as the conditions are not changed. If now the circuit from a battery or other source of electricity — for example, the excised nerve or muscle — is closed, the current entering by wire g, if this represents the anode, traverses the sul- phuric acid and mercury in the capillary and returns by the wire h. At the moment of the establishment of the current the equilibrium of forces that holds the mercury at a certain point in the capillary is disturbed, the end of the mer- cury thread moves upward with the current for a certain distance, depending on the strength of the current and the delicacy of the capillary. If the cur- rent be passed in the opposite direction the mercury will move downward a certain distance. The meniscus of contact moves up or down with the direction of the current, owing, it is supposed, to a change in the surface tension at this point. The capillary tube as used for physiological purposes is too small for the movements of the mer- cury to be detected with the eye. It is necessary to magnify it either with a microscope or a projection lantern. Ordinarily the electrometer is so made that it can be placed upon the stage of the microscope and the capillary be brought into focus at the meniscus, as shown in d, Fig. 40. By means of proper apparatus the movement can be photographed and thus a per- manent record be obtained of the direction and extent of movement of the mercury. N on-polarizable Electrodes. — In connecting a muscle or nerve to an electrometer or galvanometer it is necessary that the leading off electrodes — that is, the points of contact between the wires and the muscle or nerve — shall be iso-electrical and non-polar- izable. By iso-electrical is meant that the two elec- trodes shall have the same electrical potential, and it is obvious that the leading off electrodes must fulfill this condition approximately at least, since otherwise the current obtained from the muscle or nerve could not be attributed to differences in potential in the tissue itself; it would be shown by any other moist conductor connecting the two electrodes. Two clean platinum electrodes would fulfill this condition. A more serious difficulty is found in the polarization of metallic electrodes. Whenever a metal conductor and a liquid conductor come into contact there is apt to be polarization. This polarization expresses itself by changes at the metal poles during the passage of the current, changes of such a character that a cur- rent is set up between the poles in the opposite di- rection to the main current, thus weakening the lat- ter. This polarization current is due to the accumulation of hydrogen gas at the negative pole or cathode and of oxygen at the anode. What takes place may be represented by the following diagram, in which a current is supposed to be passing Fig. 41.— To show the structure of a non- polarizable electrode: 1, The pad of kaolin or filter paper moistened with physiological sa- line (NaCl, 0.7 per cent.) (this is placed on the tissue) ; 2, the sat- urated solution of zinc sulphate; (3) the bar of amalgamated zinc. + + Na — if + Na + Na + Na A CI CI CI CI — — s — between the poles A and C through a solution of sodium chlorid. During the passage of the current the cations, Na, with their positive charges move 96 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. toward the cathode; at the cathode the free sodium ion acts upon the water, HHO, forming NaOH and liberating hydrogen, which gives its charge to the cathode and accumulates upon it in the form of gas. The anions, CI, with their negative charges move toward the anode; there the chlorin acts upon the water, forming HC1- and liberating oxygen. It is obvious that in quantitative studies of the electrical currents of animal tissues polarization will destroy the accuracy of the results ; the demarcation current will show a diminution due not to changes in the nerve, but to physicochemical changes at the leading off electrodes. To prevent polarization du Bois-Reymond devised the non-polarizable electrodes consisting of zinc terminals immersed in zinc sulphate. Theoretically any metal in a solution of one of its salts may be used, but experience shows that the zinc-zinc sulphate electrode is most nearly perfect. Each electrode where it comes into contact with the tissue is made of one of these combinations. Various devices have been used. For instance, the electrode may be constructed as shown in the diagram (Fig. 41). A short glass tube of a bore of about 4 mms. is well cleaned — one end, which is to come into contact with the nerve, is filled, as shown, by a plug of kaolin made into a stiff putty with physiological saline (solution of NaCl, 0.7 per cent.). The kaolin should have a neutral reaction and unless good kaolin is obtainable it is better to use a plug made of clean filter paper macerated in physiological saline and packed tightly into the end of the tube. Above this plug the tube is filled in for a part of its length with a saturated solution of zinc sulphate into which is immersed a bar of amalgamated zinc with a copper wire soldered to its end. With a pair of such electrodes the conduction of the current through the nerve or muscle to the metallic part of the circuit may be represented as follows : Zn The liquid part of the circuit comes into contact with the metallic part at the junction of Zn and ZnS04. At the cathode it may be supposed that the Zn cation instead of acting upon the water and liberating hydrogen, deposits itself upon the zinc electrode; at the anode the sulphion (S04) attacks the zinc instead of the water, forming ZnS04. In this way polarization is prevented, and by the construction of the electrode the living tissue is brought into contact only with the plug of kaolin moistened with physio- logical saline. Such electrodes are indispensable in studying the electrical phe- nomena of living tissues, and also in all investigations bearing upon the polar effects during the passage of an electrical current from a battery. Ordinarily, however, when it is only desired to stimulate a nerve or muscle, metal (plat- inum) electrodes are employed. The Action Current or Negative Variation. — Du Bois-Rey- mond proved that when the excised muscle or nerve is stimulated its demarcation current suffers a diminution or negative variation. If, for instance, the excised nerve gives a demarcation current suf- ficient to cause a deflection in the galvanometer of 50 mms., then if the nerve is stimulated by a series of induction shocks the galva- nometer will show a lessened deflection, say, one of 40 mms. The negative variation in this case is equal to 10 mms., on the scale of the galvanometer used. It has been shown that this negative varia- tion is due to a current in the opposite direction whose strength, in + Zn + Zn + Na + Na + Na + Zn + Zn S04 S04 CI CI < — CI S04 S04 Zn ELECTRICAL PHENOMENA. 97 the example given, relative to that of the demarcation current is as 10 to 50 ; so that frequently the phenomenon of the negative varia- tion is known also as the action current. The explanation given for this action current is that the nerve or muscle when excited takes on an electrical condition which is negative as regards any unexcited or less excited portion of the nerve. The effect upon the demarcation current is illustrated in the accompanying diagram. The demarcation current in a nerve is led off to a galvanometer by electrodes placed at b and c. When the nerve is stimulated at a the excitation set up passes along the nerve, and wherever it may be that portion of the nerve is thrown into an electronegative con- dition. When this condition reaches a point where it can influence the galvanometer — that is, when it reaches b, it will diminish the difference in potential that exists between b and c, and therefore reduce the current flowing from b to c. _f_ Bernstein* has shown that this neg- ative condition moves in the form of a wave. That is, at any point the nega- tivity grOWS to a Fi? 42 _ Schema to indicate the method of detecting maximum and then *ne acti°n current in a stimulated excised nerve: b and c, . . the leading off electrodes, one on the longitudinal, one on diminishes. More- the cut surface; the demarcation current passes through . the galvanometer, g, in the direction of the arrows; a, stimu- OVer, it travels at a lating electrodes from induction coil; the stimulus causes a _j£»., i • , negative condition, — which passes along the nerve; when aennite VelOCltV this reaches 6 it causes a partial reversal of the demarca- whirh is Pfl, most of the conduction from neuron to neuron, along sensory as well as motor paths, is made with the aid of such structures, the dendrites being the receptive or sensory organ and the axon the motor apparatus. The Golgi cells of the second type (Fig. 56) are relatively less numerous and important. They are characterized by the fact that the axon process instead of forming a nerve fiber splits into a great number of branches within the gray matter. Assuming that in such cells the distinction be- tween the axon and the den- drites is well made and that as in the other type the dendrites form the receiving and the axon the discharging apparatus, these cells would seem to have a distributive function. The impulse that they receive may be trans- Fig. 57. — Normal anterior horn cell (Warrington), showing the Nissl granules in the cell and dendrites : a, The axon. mitted to one or many neurons. They are sometimes spoken of as intermediate or association cells. Internal Structure of the Nerve Cell.— Within the body of the nerve cell itself the striking features of physiological significance are, first, the arrangement of the neurofibrils, and, secondly, the PROPERTIES OF THE NERVE CELL. 127 presence of a materia] in the form of granules, rods, or masses which stains readily with the basic anilin dyes, such as methylene blue, thionin, or toluidin blue. This latter substance is spoken of as the "chromophile substance," tigroid, or more frequently as Nissl's granules, after the histologist who first studied it success- fully. These masses or granules are found in the dendrites as well as in the cell, but are absent from the axon (see Fig. 57). Little is known of their composition or significance, but their presence or ab- sence is in many cases characteristic of the physiological condition of the cell. After lesions or injuries of the neuron the material may become dissolved and diffused through the cell or may decrease in amount or disappear, and it seems probable, therefore, that it repre- sents a store of nutritive material (Fig. 58). The non-staining material of the cell, according to most recent observers, contains neurofibrils which are continued out into the processes, dendrites as &'"*$£ Fig. 58. — Anterior horn cell fourteen days after section of the anterior root (Warring- tori) : To show the change in the nucleus and the Nissl granules, beginning chromatolysis. well as axons. These fibrils may be regarded as the conducting structure along which passes the nerve impulse. The arrangement of these fibrils within the cell is not completely known, the results obtained varying with the methods employed. A matter of far- reaching importance on the physiological side is the question of the existence of an extracellular nervous network. Most recent histologists agree in the belief that there is a delicate network surrounding the cells and their protoplasmic processes. This pericellular net or Golgi's net is claimed by some to be a ner- vous structure connecting with the neurofibrils inside the cell and forming not only a bond of union between the neurons, but possibly also an important intercellular nervous structure that may play an important role in the functions of the nerve centers. This view is represented schematically in Fig. 59. According to others, this network around and outside the cells is a supporting tissue simply that takes no part in the activity of the nerve units. 128 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. General Physiology of the Nerve Cell. — Modern physiologists have considered the cell body of the neuron, including the den- drites, as the source of the energy displayed by the nervous system, and it has been assumed that this energy arises from chemical changes in the nerve cell, as the energy liberated by the muscle arises from the chemical changes in its substance. It would follow from this standpoint that evidences of chemical activity should be obtained from the cells and that the elements should exhibit the phenomenon of fatigue. Regarding this latter point, it is believed in physiology that the nerve cells fatigue readily. The nerve centers show fatigue as the result of continuous activity, as is evident from our personal experience in prolonged intellectual or emo- tional activity and as is implied in the necessity of sleep for re- Fig. 59. — Bethe's schema to indicate the connections of the pericellular network: Rz, A sensory cell in the posterior root ganglion ; the fibrils in the branch that runs to the cord are indicated as connecting directly with the pericellular network of the motor cells, Gz. cuperation and the rapidity with which functional activity is lost on withdrawal of the blood supply. Objectively, also, it has been shown in the ergographic experiments (see p. 45) that the well- known fatigue of the neuromuscular apparatus probably affects the nerve centers as well as the muscle. Assuming that the nerve cells are the effective agent in the nerve centers, such facts indicate that they are susceptible to fatigue. But we have no very direct proof that this property is shown universally by the nerve cells nor any indication of the probable differences in this regard shown by nerve cells in different parts of the central nervous system. It seems probable that under normal conditions — that is, under the influence of what we may call minimal stimuli — some portions of the nerve centers remain in more or less constant activity during PROPERTIES OF THE NERVE CELL. 129 the day without showing a marked degree of fatigue, just as our muscles remain in a more or less continuous state of tonic con- traction throughout the waking period at least. Doubtless when the stimulation is stronger the fatigue is more marked, because the processes of repair in the nerve centers can not then keep pace with the processes of consumption of material. Evidence of a probable chemical change in the nerve cells during activity is found also in the readiness with which the gray matter of the nervous system takes on an acid reaction.* In the fresh resting state it is probably alkaline, but after death it quickly shows an acid reaction, due, it is said, to the production of lactic acid. Its resemblance to the muscle in this respect leads to the inference that in functional activity acid is also produced. Mosso states that in the brain increased mental activity is accompanied by a rise in the temperature of the brain. f His experiments were made upon individuals with an opening in the skull through which a delicate thermometer could be inserted so as to lie in contact with brain. So also the facts briefly mentioned in regard to the Nissl granules give some corroborative evidence that the activity of the nervous system is accompanied by and probably caused hy a chemical change within the cells, since the excessive activity of the nerve cells seems to be accompanied by some change in these granules, and in abnormal conditions associated with loss of func- tional activity the granules undergo chromatolysis, — that is, they are disintegrated and dissolved. Obvious histological changes which imply, of course, a change in chemical structure, have been observed by a number of investigators. J All seem to agree that activity of the tissue, whether normal or induced by artificial stimulation, may cause visible changes in the appearance of the cell and its nucleus. Activity within normal limits may cause an increase in the size of the cell together with a diminution in the stainable (Nissl) substance, and excessive activity a diminution in size of the cell and the nucleus, the formation of vacuoles in the cell body, and a marked effect upon the stainable material. Hodge has shown that in birds, for instance, the spinal ganglion cells of a swallow killed at nightfall after a day of activity exhibit a marked loss of substance as compared with similar cells from an animal killed in the early morning (Fig. 60). It must be remembered, however, that our knowledge of the nature of the chemical changes that occur in the cell during activity is very meager. Presumably carbon dioxid and lactic acid are * Langendorff, " Centralbl. f. d. med. Wiss.," 1886. See also Halliburton, " The Croonian Lectures on the Chemical Side of Nervous Activity," 1901. t Mosso, "Die Temperatur des Gehirns," 1894. t See especially Hodge, " Journal of Morphology," 7, 95, 1892, and 9, 1, 1894. 9 130 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. formed as in muscle, and we know that oxygen is consumed. Enough is known perhaps to justify the general view that the energy exhibited by the nervous system is derived, in the long run, from a metabolism of material in the nerve cells, a metabolism which consists essentially in the splitting and oxidation of the complex substances in the protoplasm of the cell. Summation of the Effects of Stimuli. — In a muscle a series of stimuli will cause a greater amount of shortening than can be obtained from a single stimulus of the same strength. In this case the effects of the stimuli are summated, one contraction taking place on top of another, or to put it in another way, the muscle while in a condition of contraction from one stimulus is made to Fig. 60. — Spinal ganglion cells from English sparrows, to show the daily variation in the appearance of the cells due to normal activity: A. Appearance of cells at the end of an active day; B, appearance of cells in the morning after a night's rest. The cytoplasm is filled with clear, lenticular masses, which are much more evident in the rested cells than in those fatigued. — (Hodge.) contract still more by the following stimulus. In the nerve fiber such a phenomenon has not been demonstrated. The strength of the nerve impulse can be determined only by means of the effect on the end-organ, — e. g.,the muscle, — in which case the properties of the end-organ must be taken into account, or by the aid of the electrical response. Now, when a nerve is stimulated so rapidly that the second stimulus falls into the nerve before the electrical change due to the first stimulus has passed off, the second stimulus, instead of adding its effect to that of the first, simply has no effect at all; it finds the nerve unheritable.* According to this result, we should expert that a summation of the effects of rapidly *Gotch and Burch, ''Journal of Physiology," 24, 410, 1899. PROPERTIES OF THE NERVE CELL. 131 following stimuli is not possible in the case of the nerve fiber. In the nerve cell, on the contrary, it is usually taught that the power of summation is a characteristic property, although it may be said that the proofs for this belief are not very direct. It is pointed out that, while a single stimulus applied to a sensory nerve may be ineffective in producing a reflex response from the central nervous system, a series of such stimuli will call forth a reaction. In this case it is assumed that the effects of the suc- ceeding stimuli are summated within the nerve cells through which the reflex takes place, and, generally speaking, it is assumed in physiology that the nerve centers are adapted by their power of summation to respond to a series of stimuli or to continuous stimu- lation. The best examples of this kind of action are obtained perhaps from sensory nerves, in which case we judge of the intensity of the cell activity by the concomitant sensation, or by a reflex response. Response of the Nerve Cell to Varying Rates of Stimula- tion.— The various parts of the neuromuscular apparatus — namely, the nerve cell, the nerve fiber, and the muscle fiber — have different degrees of responsiveness to repeated stimuli, and this responsiveness varies, moreover, for the different kinds of mus- cles and of nerve fibers, and, probably for the different kinds of nerve cells. The motor cells of the brain discharge their motor impulses normally at a rate probably of about 10 per second (see p. 43), and it is very interesting to find that, if these cells are stimulated artificially, their rate of motor discharge does not keep pace with that of the stimulation employed, but occurs at about the same rate as the normal, — namely, at about 10 per second. Thus, Horsley and Schafer* found that in monkeys, dogs, cats, and rabbits, stimulation of the motor regions of the cortex or the motor cells in the cord gave tetanic muscular contractions, which from their graphic records were evidently composed of simple contractions following at an average rate of 10 per second, although the stimuli applied to the center might vary in rate from 10 to 50 per second. Similar results by a somewhat different method were obtained by Broca and Richet.f These authors point out, moreover, that no mental act can be repeated more rapidly (on the average) than 10 times per second. If one, for instance, attempts to think a series of syllables or words in a given phrase the maximum of rapidity with which each syllable can be clearly thought is at the rate named. The authors last named believe, therefore, that the minimal duration for an intellectual act is probably approximately about -^ of a second. These facts, so * Horsley and Schafer, " Journal of Physiology," 7, 96, 1886. f Broca and Richet, "Journal de physiol. norm, et pathol.," 1897, p. 864. 132 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. far as they go, would indicate that in the cerebrum and the cord the nerve cells react with a certain rhythm. The Refractory Period of the Nerve Cell. — The peculiar rhythm of the active nerve cell just referred to in the para- graph above is explained most satisfactorily by an assumption first used in connection with the rhythmical beat of the heart. As will be explained more fully in the section on the physiology of the heart, it has been found that after the contraction of the heart begins it is unirritable to artificial stimuli, and that its irritability is recovered during the period of rest, — the diastole. The heart has, therefore, alternate periods of irritability and unirritability. The latter phase, the condition in which the heart muscle will not respond to stimulation, is known as the refractory period, or refractory phase. Inasmuch as it appears immediately after the contraction, it is usually explained as being due to some product of the chemical reaction causing the contraction, — in fact, a state of temporary fatigue. A similar conception has been applied to the nerve cell. The experiments cited in the preceding para- graph would indicate that, after the discharge of an impulse, the cell falls into a refractory phase for a period of time lasting about 0.1 sec. The idea is a convenient one, although we have no explana- tion of what is the immediate cause of this temporary loss of irrita- bility. Reasoning from analogy with the muscle, we might suppose that in this case also it is due to some product of the chemical reaction that is assumed to underlie nervous activity. Using this terminol- ogy, it is probable that the cells in different parts of the nervous system may have different refractory periods. In the case of the normal nerve fiber (see p. 112) it will be recalled that the refractory period is very brief, — say, 0.006 sec, — but varies with the condi- tion of the fiber, since in the narcotized fiber it may be as much as 0.1 sec. CHAPTER VII. REFLEX ACTIONS. Definition and Historical. — By a reflex action we mean the involuntary production of activity in some peripheral tissue through the efferent nerve fibers connected with it in consequence of a stimulation of afferent nerve fibers. The conversion of the sensory or afferent impulse into a motor or efferent impulse is effected in the nerve centers, and may be totally unconscious as well as invol- untary,— for instance, the emptying of the gall-bladder during digestion, or it may be accompanied by consciousness of the act, as, for example, in the winking reflex when the eye is touched. The application of the term reflex to such acts seems to have been made first by Descartes* (1649), on the analogy of the reflection of light, the sensory effect in these cases being reflected back, so to speak, as a motor effect. The attention of the early physiologists was directed to these involuntary movements and many instances were collected, both in man and the lower animals. Their invol- untary character was emphasized by the discovery that similar movements are given by decapitated animals, — frogs, eels, etc. Some of the earlier physiologists thought that the reflex might occur in the anastomoses of the nerve trunks, but a convincing proof that the central nervous system is the place of reflection was given by Whytt (1751). He showed that in a decapitated frog the reflex movements are abolished if the spinal cord is destroyed. Modern interest in the subject was excited by the numerous works of Marshall Hall (1832-57), who contributed a number of new facts with regard to such acts, and formulated a view, not now accepted, that these reflexes are mediated by a special set of fibers — the excitomotor fibers. In describing reflexes the older physiologists had in mind only reflex movements, but at the present time we recognize that the reflex act may affect not only the muscles, — voluntary, involuntary, and cardiac, — but also the glands. We have to deal with reflex secretions as well as reflex movements. The Reflex Arc. — It is implied in the definition of a reflex that both sensory and motor paths are concerned in the act. Ac- * See Eckhard, " Geschichte der Entwickelung der Lehre von den Reflex- erscheinungen," " Beitrage zur Anatomie u. Physiologie," Giessen, 1881, vol. ix. 133 134 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. cording to the neuron theory, therefore, the simplest reflex arc must consist of two neurons: the sensory neuron, whose cell body lies in one of the posterior root or cranial nerve ganglia, and a motor neuron, whose nerve cell lies in the anterior horn of gray matter of the cord or in the motor nucleus of a cranial nerve. The reflex arc for the spinal cord is represented in Fig. 61. The arc may, however, be more complex. The sensory fibers entering through the posterior roots may pass upward through the entire length of the cord to end in the medulla and on the way give off a number of collaterals as represented in Fig. 62, or they may make connections with intermediate cells which, in turn, are connected with one or more motor neurons (Fig. 63). According Fig. 61. — Schema to show the connection between the neuron of the posterior root and the neuron of the anterior root, — the reflex arc. to these schemata, one sensory fiber may establish reflex connections with a number of different motor fibers, or, a fact which must be borne in mind in studying some of the well-known reflex activities of the cord and medulla especially, a sensory fiber carrying an impulse which eventually reaches the cortex of the cerebrum and gives rise to a conscious sensation may, by means of its collaterals, connect with motor nuclei in the cord or medulla and thus at the same time give origin to involuntary and even unconscious re- flexes. Painful stimulation of the skin, for example, may give us a conscious sensation of pain and at the same time reflexly stimulate the vasomotor center and cause a constriction of the small arteries. The fact that in this case two distinct events occur does not necessitate the assumption that the impulses from the REFLEX ACTIONS. 135 skin are carried to the cord by two different varieties of fibers. It may well be that one variety of sensory neuron, the so-called pain fibers, effects both results, because of the opportunities in the cord for connections with different groups of nerve cells. The Reflex Frog. — The motor reflexes from the spinal cord can be studied most successfully upon a frog in which the brain has been destroyed or whose head has been cut off. After such an operation the animal may for a time suffer from shock, but a vigorous animal will usually recover and after some hours will Fig. 62. — Kolliker's schema to show the direct reflex arc. It shows the pos- terior root fiber (black) entering the cord, dividing in Y. and connecting with motor cells (red) by means of collater- als. Fig. 63. — Kolliker's schema to show the reflex arc with intercal- ated tract cells. Posterior root fiber, black; intercalated tract cell, blue; motor cells, red. exhibit reflex movements that are most interesting. The funda- mental characteristics of reflex movements in their relations to the place, intensity, and quality of the stimulus can be studied with more ease upon an animal whose cord is thus severed from the brain than upon a normal animal. In the latter case the connec- tions in the nervous system are more complex and the reactions are therefore less simple and less easily kept constant. Spinal Reflex Movements. — The reflex movements obtained from the spinal cord or from parts of the central nervous system 136 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. may be divided into three groups by characteristics that are physio- logically significant. These classes are: (1) Simple reflexes, or those in which a single muscle is affected. The best example of this group is perhaps the winking reflex, in which only the orbic- ularis palpebrarum is concerned. (2) Co-ordinated reflexes, in which a number of muscles react with their contractions so grad- uated as to time and extent as to produce an orderly and useful movement. (3) Convulsive reflexes, such as are seen in spasms, in which a number of muscles — perhaps all the muscles — are con- tracted convulsively, without co-ordination and with the pro- duction of disorderly and useless movements. Of these groups, the co-ordinated reflexes are by far the most interesting. They can be obtained to perfection from the reflex frog. In such an animal no spontaneous movements occur if the sensory surfaces are entirely protected from stimulation. A sudden stimulus, however, of sufficient strength applied to any part of the skin will give a definite and practically invariable response in a movement which has the appearance of an intentional effort to escape from or remove the stimulus. If the toe is pinched the foot is withdrawn — in a gentle manner if the stimulus is light, more rapidly and violently, but still in a purposeful and co-ordinated fashion, if the stimulus is strong. If the animal is suspended and various spots on its skin are stimulated by the application of bits of paper moistened with dilute acetic acid the animal will make a neat and skillful movement of the corresponding leg to remove the stimulating body. The reactions may be varied in a number of ways, and in all cases the striking features of the reflex response are, first, the purposeful character of the movement, and, second, the almost mechanical exactness with which a definite stimulus will give a definite re- sponse. This definite relationship holds only for the external integument, the skin and its organs. It is obvious, in fact, that a muscular response can be effective only for stimuli originating from the external surface. Stimuli from the interior of the body exert their reactions, for the most part, upon the plain musculature and the glands. The convulsive reflexes may be produced by two different means: (1) By very intense sensory stimulation. The reflex response in this case overflows, as it were, into all the motor paths. A variation of this method is seen in the well- known convulsive reaction that follows tickling. In this case the stimulus, although not intense from an objective standpoint, is obviously violent from the standpoint of its effectiveness in sending into the central nervous system a series of maximal sensory im- pulses. (2) By heightening the irritability of the central nervous system. Upon the reflex frog this effect is obtained most readily by the use of strychnin. A little strychnin injected under the REFLEX ACTIONS. 137 skin is soon absorbed and its effect is shown at first by a greater sensitiveness to cutaneous stimulation, the slightest touch to the foot causing its withdrawal. Soon, however, the response, instead of being orderly and purposeful, becomes convulsive. A mere touch of the skin or a current of air will throw every muscle into contraction, and the extensors being stronger than the flexors the animal's body becomes rigid in extension at every stimulation. The explanation usually given for this result is that the strychnin, acting upon some part of the nerve cells, increases greatly their irritability, so that when a stimulus is sent into the central nervous system along any sensory path from the skin it apparently radiates throughout the cord and acts upon all the motor cells. This latter supposition leads to the interesting conclusion that all the various motor neurons of the cord must be in physiological connection, either direct or indirect, with all the neurons supplying the cutaneous sur- face. The further fact that under normal conditions the effect of a given sensory stimulus is manifested only on a limited and practically constant number of the motor neurons seems to imply, therefore, that normally the paths to these neurons are more direct and the resistance, if we may use a somewhat figurative term, is less than by other possible paths. Muscular spasms are observed under a number of pathological conditions, — for instance, in hydrophobia. We are at liberty to assume in such cases that the toxins produced by the disease affect the irritability of the cells in much the same way as the strychnin. Theory of Co-ordinated Reflexes. — The purposeful character of the co-ordinated reflexes in the frog gives the impression to the observer of a conscious choice of movements on the part of the brainless animal. Most physiologists, however, are content to see in these reactions only an expression of the automatic activity of a mechanism. It is assumed that the sensory impulses from any part of the skin find, on reaching the cord, that the paths to a certain group of motor neurons are more direct and offer less resistance than any others. It is along these paths that the reflex will take place, and we may further assume that these paths of least resistance, as they have been called, are in part preformed and in part are laid down by the repeated experiences of the indi- vidual. That is, in each animal a definite structure may be sup- posed to exist in the cord; each sensory neuron is connected with a group of motor neurons, to some of them more directly than to others, and we may imagine, therefore, a system of reflex apparatuses ■or mechanisms which when properly stimulated will react always in the same way. And, indeed, in spite of the purposeful character of the reflexes under consideration their automaton-like regularity is an indication that their production is due to a fixed mechanical 138 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. arrangement. Whether or not the reactions of the nervous system in such cases are accompanied by any degree of consciousness can not be proved or disproved, but the assumption of such an accom- paniment does not seem necessary to explain the reaction. Spinal Reflexes in the Mammals. — Experiments upon the lower mammals, such as the dog, show that co-ordinated reflex move- ments may be obtained from the lower portion of the cord after severance of its connections with the brain. The spinal cord may be severed, for instance, in the thoracic region and the animal be kept alive and in good condition for an indefinite period. In such an animal reflex movements of the hind legs or tail may be obtained readily from slight sensory stimulation of the skin. The knee-jerk and similar so-called deep reflexes are also retained. But it is evident that these movements are not so complete nor so distinctly purposeful as in the frog. The muscles of the body supplied by the isolated part of the cord retain, however, a normal irritability and exhibit no wasting. In man, on the contrary, it is stated that after complete section of the cord the deep reflexes, such as the knee-jerk, as well as the skin reflexes, are very quickly lost. The muscles undergo wasting and soon lose their irritability.* The monkeys exhibit in this respect a condition that is somewhat intermediate between that of the dog and man. It seems evident from these facts that in the lower animals like the frog a much greater degree of independent activity is exhibited by the cord than in the more highly developed animals. According to the degree of development, the control of the muscles is assumed more and more by the higher portions of the nervous system, and the spinal cord becomes less important as a series of reflex centers, its func- tions being more dependent upon its connections with the higher centers. Dependence of Co-ordinated Reflexes upon the Excitation of the Normal Sensory Endings, — It is an interesting fact that when a nerve trunk is stimulated directly in a reflex frog — the sciatic nerve, for instance — the reflex movements are disorderly and quite unlike those obtained by stimulating the skin. It is said that if the skin be loosened and the nerve twigs arising from it are stimulated, an operation that is quite possible in the frog, the re- sponse is again a disorderly reflex, whereas the same fibers stimu- lated through the skin give an orderly, co-ordinated movement. The difference in response in these cases is probably not due to any peculiarity in the nature of the sensory impulses originating in the nerve endings of the skin, but more likely to a difference in their strength and arrangement. When one stimulates a sensory nerve trunk directly, — the ulnar nerve at the elbow in ourselves, for in- * See Collier, * Brain," 1904, p. 38. REFLEX ACTIONS. 139 stance, — the resulting sensations are markedly different from those obtained by stimulating the skin areas supplied by the same nerve; we have little or no sensations of touch or temperature, only pain and a peculiar tingling in the fingers. In such an experiment the stimulus applied to the trunk affects more or less equally all the contained fibers, whereas in stimulation of the skin itself the effect upon the cutaneous fibers of pressure-, temperature, or pain pre- dominates and presumably it is these fibers that normally are con- nected in an efficient way with the reflex machinery in the nerve centers. Reflex Time. — Since in a reflex movement the nerve centers are involved a determination of the total time between the appli- cation of the stimulus and the beginning of the response gives a means of ascertaining the time element concerned in the central processes. Helmholtz, who first made experiments of this kind, stated that the time required within the nerve centers might be as much as twelve times as great as that estimated for the conduction along the motor and sensory nerves involved in the reflex. Most observers state that the time within the center varies with the strength of the stimulus, being less the stronger the stimulus. It varies also with the condition of the nerve centers, being diminished by fatigue and other conditions that depress the irritability of the nerve cells. By reflex time or reduced reflex time we may designate the time required for the processes in the center, — that is, the total time less that required for transmission of the impulse along the motor and sensory fibers and the latent period of the muscle con- traction. For the frog this is estimated as varying between 0.008 and 0.015 sec. In man the reflex time usually quoted is that given by Exner for the winking of the eye. He stimulated one lid electrically and recorded the reflex movement of the lid of the other eye. The total time for the reflex was, on an average, from 0.0578 sec. to 0.0662 sec. He estimated that the time for transmission of the impulse along the sensory and motor paths, together with the latent period of the muscle, amounted to 0.0107 sec. So that the true reflex time from his determinations varied between 0.0471 and 0.0555 sec. Mayhew,* using a more elaborate method, obtained for the total time a mean figure equal to 0.0420 sec. If Exner's correction is applied then the true reflex time according to this de- termination is equal to 0.0313 sec. Inhibition of Reflexes. — One of the most fundamental facts regarding spinal reflexes is the demonstration that they can be depressed or suppressed entirely — that is, inhibited — by other im- pulses reaching the same part of the spinal cord. The most sig- * Mayhew, " Journal of Exp. Medicine," 2, 35, 1897. 140 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. nificant experiment in this connection is that made by Setschenow.* If in a frog the entire brain or the cerebral hemispheres are re- moved, then stimulation of the exposed cut surface — for instance, by crystals of sodium chlorid — will depress greatly or perhaps inhibit entirely the usual spinal reflexes that may be obtained by cutaneous stimulation. On removal of the stimulating substance from the cut surface by washing with a stream of physiological saline (solution of sodium chlorid, 0.7 per cent.) the reflex activities of the cord are again exhibited in a normal way. This experiment accords with many facts which indicate that the brain may inhibit the activities of the spinal centers. In the reflex from tickling, for instance, we know that by a voluntary act we can repress the muscular movements up to a certain point; so also the limited control of the action of the respiratory and micturition centers is a phenomenon of the same character. To explain such acts we may assume the existence of a definite set of inhibitory fibers, arising in parts of the brain and distributed to the spinal cord, whose function is that of controlling the activities of the spinal centers. In view of the fact, however, that there is no independent proof of the existence of a separate set of inhibitory fibers within the central nervous system — that is, a set of fibers whose specific energy is that of inhibition — it is preferable to speak simply of the inhibitory influence of the brain upon the cord, leaving unde- cided the question as to whether this influence is exerted through a special set of fibers, or is brought about by some variation in the time relations, intensity, or quality of the nerve impulses. Regarding the fact, however, there can be no question, and it constitutes a most important factor in the interaction of the dif- ferent parts of the nervous system. It is possible that this factor may explain why a normal frog gives reflexes that are so much less constant and less predictable than one with its brain removed. A similar inhibition of spinal reflexes may be obtained by simul- taneous stimulation of two different parts of the skin. The usual reflex from pinching the toe of one leg may be inhibited in part or completely by simultaneous stimulation of the other leg or direct electrical stimulation of an exposed nerve trunk. A similar interference is illustrated perhaps in the well-known device of inhibiting an act of sneezing by a strong sensory stimulation from some part of the skin, — for instance, by pressing upon the upper lip. Inhibition of spinal reflexes by such means is not so constant nor so effective as by stimulation of the central paths, but it forms an interesting phenomenon which must be taken into account in any hypothesis of the nature of inhibition that may be proposed. * Setschenow, u Physiologische Studien uber d. Hemmungs-Mechanismen f. d. Reflexthatigkeit im Gehirn d. Frosches," Berlin, 1863. REFLEX ACTIONS. 141 A brief statement of the more or less unsatisfactory theories of inhibition is given in connection with the inhibitory action of the vagus nerve on the heart beat (see p. 512). It should be added, however, in this connection that stimulation of the cord, and probably of other parts of the nervous system, from two different sources may result not only in an inhibition of the reflex normally occurring from one of the stimuli, but under some circumstances may give an augmentation or reinforcement of the reflex. The most striking example of this augmenting effect is given below in the paragraph upon the knee-kick. Influence of the Condition of the Cord on its Reflex Ac- tivities.— The time and extent of the reflex responses may be altered greatly by various influences, particularly by the action of drugs. The effect in such cases is usually upon the nerve centers, — that is, upon the cells themselves or upon the connections between the terminal arborization and the dendrites — the process of conduc- tion within the sensory and motor fibers being less easily affected. A convenient method of studying such influences is that employed by Ttirck. In this method the reflex frog is suspended, and the tip of the longest toe is immersed to a definite point in a solution of sulphuric acid of a strength of 0.1 to 0.2 per cent. If the time between the immersion and the reflex withdrawal of the foot is noted by a metronome, or by a record upon a kymograph, it will be found to be quite constant, provided the conditions are kept uniform. If the average time for this reflex is obtained from a series of observations it is possible to inject various substances — such as strychnin, chloroform, potassium bromid, quinin, etc. — under the skin, and after absorption has taken place to determine the effect by a new series of observations. So far as drugs are concerned the results of such experiments belong rather to pharma- cology than to physiology. The method in some cases brings out an interesting difference in the effects of various kinds of stimulation. Strychnin, for instance, as was stated above, increases greatly the delicacy of the reaction to pressure stimulation. At one stage in its action before the convulsive responses are obtained the threshold stimulus is greatly lowered, — mere contact with the toes causes a rapid retraction of the leg; whereas in the normal reflex frog a relatively large pressure is necessary to obtain a similar response. At this stage in the action of the strychnin the effect of the acid stimulus, on the contrary, may be markedly weakened so far as the time element is concerned. If the action of the strychnin is not too rapid, it is usually possible to find a point at which the time for the reflex is diminished, but this effect quickly disappears and the period between stimulus and response becomes markedly lengthened at a time when the slightest mechanical stimulation gives 142 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. a rapid reflex movement. This paradoxical result may depend pos- sibly upon the variety of nerve fiber stimulated by the two kinds of stimuli or may be connected with the fact that the acid stimuli may bring about inhibitory as well as excitatory processes in the cord. Reflexes from Other Parts of the Nervous System. — Nu- merous typical reflexes are known to occur in the brain. The reflex effects upon the important centers in the medulla, such as the vasomotor center, the respiratory center, and the cardio- inhibitory center, the winking of the eye, sneezing, the light reflex upon the sphincter muscle of the iris, and many other similar cases might be enumerated. All of these reactions will be described and discussed in their proper places. The conscious reactions of the brain are not included among the reflexes by virtue of the defi- nition which lays stress upon the involuntary characteristic of the reflex response, but it should be remembered that, so far as the nervous mechanism is concerned, these conscious reactions do not differ from the true reflexes. When we voluntarily move a limb the movement is guided and controlled by sensory impulses from the muscles put into action. The fibers of muscle sense from these muscles convey sensory impulses through a chain of neurons to the cortex of the brain and there the impulses doubtless affect and set into action the motor neuron through which the movement is effected. So far as we know, the discharges from the efferent neuron of the brain are not really automatic, but are conditioned or originated by stimuli from other neurons ; so that the activities of the brain are carried on by a mechanism of one neuron acting on another, just as in the case of the reflex arc. The added feature of a psychical factor, a reaction in consciousness, enables us to draw a line of distinction between these activities and those of so-called pure reflexes; but the distinction is perhaps one of convenience only, for, although the extremes may be far enough apart to suit the definition, many intermediate instances may be found which are difficult to classify. All skilled movements, for instance, such as walking, singing, dancing, bicycle riding, and the like, — although in the beginning obviously effected by voluntary co-ordination, nevertheless in the end, in proportion to the skill obtained, become more or less entirely reflex, — that is, involuntary. In learning such movements one must, as the saying goes, establish his reflexes, and the result can hardly be understood otherwise than by suppos- ing that the continual adjustment of certain sensory impulses to certain co-ordinated movements results in the formation of a more or less complex reflex arc, a set of paths of least resistance. Reflexes through Peripheral Ganglia — Axon Reflexes. — Many attempts have been made by physiologists to ascertain REFLEX ACTIONS. 143 whether or not reflexes can occur through the peripheral nerve ganglia, particularly those belonging to the sympathetic system. With regard to the posterior root ganglia, it may be said that no reflexes are possible through them. If the posterior root connect- ing such a ganglion to the cord is severed stimulation of the sensory area supplied by these ganglia causes no reflex response. Indeed, according to our conception of the mechanism of a reflex, the pos- terior root ganglia could not serve as reflex centers: they contain apparently no efferent neurons. In the ganglia of the sympathetic nerve and its appendages and in the similar ganglia contained in many of the organs the nerve cells have dendritic processes, and, so far as their histology is concerned, it would seem possible' that in any ganglion of this type there might be sensory and motor neu- rons so connected as to make the ganglion an independent reflex center. Numerous experiments have been made to determine experimentally whether reflexes can be ob- tained through such ganglia. Perhaps the most successful of these experiments have been made upon the inferior mesenteric ganglion. This ganglion may be isolated from all connections with the central nervous system and left attached to the bladder through the two hypogastric nerves (see schema, Fig. 105). If now one of these nerves is cut. and the central stump is stimulated a contraction of the bladder follows. Obviously in this case the impulse has traveled to the ganglion and down the other hypogastric nerve; the reaction has every appearance of being a true reflex. Nevertheless, Langley and Ander- son,* who have studied the matter with espe- cial care, are convinced that in this and similar cases we have to do with what they call pseudoreflexes or axon reflexes. The idea under- lying this term may be explained in this way: Every sympathetic ganglion is connected with the central nervous system, brain, and cord, by efferent spinal fibers, preganglionic fibers, which terminate by arborization around the dendrites of the sympathetic cells. The efferent fibers arising from the latter may be designated as post- ganglionic fibers. These authors give reasons to believe that any one preganglionic fiber, a, Fig. 64, may connect by collaterals with * Langley and Anderson, "Journal of Physiology," 16, 410, 1894. Fig. 64. — Schema to show idea of an axon re- flex : The preganglionic fiber, a, sends branches to two postganglionic fibers, b, c. If stimulated at x the impulse passes backward in a direction the reverse of normal and falling into b and c gives a pseudoreflex effect. 144 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. several sympathetic cells. If such a fiber were stimulated at x, then the impulse passing back along the axon in a direction the reverse of normal would stimulate cells b and c, giving effects that are apparently reflex, but which differ from true reflexes in that the stimulating axon belongs to a motor neuron. Under normal circumstances it is not probable that an effect of this kind can be produced. The Tonic Activity of the Spinal Cord. — In addition to the definite reflex activities of the cord, each traceable to a distinct sensory stimulus, there is evidence to show that many of its motor neurons are in that state of more or less continuous activity which we designate as tonic activity or tonus. There is abundant reason for this belief in regard to many of the special centers of the cord and brain, such as the vasomotor center, the center for the sphinc- ter muscle of the iris, the centers for the sphincter muscles of the bladder, the anus, etc. But the evidence includes the motor neurons to the voluntary as well as the involuntary musculature. In a decapitated frog the muscles take a definite position, and Brondgeest showed that if such an animal is suspended, after cut- ting the sciatic plexus in one leg, the leg on the uninjured side takes a more flexed position. The explanation offered for this result is that the muscles on the sound side are being innervated by the motor neurons of the cord. Inasmuch as a result of this kind cannot be obtained from a frog whose skin has been removed, or in one in which the posterior roots have been severed it seems evident that this tonic discharge from the motor neurons is due to a constant inflow of impulses along the sensory paths. The muscle tonus, in other words, is really a reflex tonus, which differs from ordinary reflex movements only in the absence of a sudden, visible contraction and in the more or less continuous character of the innervation. In the section on animal heat the importance of this constant innervation of the muscles as a source of heat is further emphasized. The idea of a more or less continuous but varying activity of the centers in the brain and cord in consequence of the continuous inflow of impulses along the sensory paths fits in very well with many facts observed in the peripheral organs, — facts that will be referred to from time to time as the physiology of these organs is considered. Effects of Removal of the Spinal Cord. — Numerous investi- gators have sectioned the cord partly or completely at various levels. The general results of these experiments as regards loss of sensation or voluntary movement are described in the next section treating of the cord as a path of conduction to and from the brain. But attention may be called here to some of the gen- REFLEX ACTIONS. 145 eral results obtained by Goltz* in some remarkable experiments in which the entire cord was removed with the exception of the cervical region and a small portion of the upper thoracic. In making this experiment it was necessary to perform the operation in several steps. That is, the cord was first sectioned in the upper thoracic region and then in successive operations the lower tho- racic, lumbar, and sacral regions were removed completely. Very great care was necessary in the treatment of the animals after these operations, but some survived and lived for long periods, the digestive, circulatory, and excretory organs performing their functions in a normal manner. The muscles of the hind limbs and trunk, however, underwent complete atrophy, owing to the destruction of their motor nerves. The blood-vessels also were paralyzed after the first operations, but gradually their muscu- lature again recovered tone, showing that, although under normal conditions the tonic contraction of the vessels is under the in- fluence of nerves arising from the cord, this tone may be re-estab- lished in time after the severance of all spinal connections. Some of the specific results of these experiments, bearing upon the re- flexes of defecation, micturition, and parturition, will be described later. Attention may be called here to the general results illustrating the general functions of the cord. In the first place, there was, of course, a total paralysis of volun- tary movement in the muscles innervated normally through the parts of the cord removed, and a complete loss of sensation in the same regions, particularly of cutaneous and muscular sensibility. In the second place, the visceral organs, including the blood-vessels, — that is, the vegetative or unconscious organs, — were shown to be much more independent of the direct control of the central nervous system. While these organs in the experiments under consideration were still in connection with the sympathetic ganglia and in part with the brain through the vagi, still their connections with the central nervous system, particularly as regards their sensory paths and the innervation of the blood-vessels, were in largest part destroyed. The immediate effect of this destruction would have been the death of the animal if the artificial care of the observer had not replaced, in the beginning, the normal control exercised by the nervous sys- tem through the spinal nerves; but later this careful nursing was not required. While these organs, therefore, are capable of a certain amount of independent activity and co-ordination, they are normally controlled through the various reflex activities of the brain and cord. In the third place, it is noteworthy that the adaptability of the cordless portion of the animal was distinctly * Goltz and Ewald, " Pfliiger's Archiv fur die gesammte Physiologie," 63, 362, 1896. 10 146 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. less than normal. Its power of preserving a constant body tem- perature was more limited than in the normal animal, and the susceptibility to inflammatory disturbances in the visceral organs was greatly increased. It seems evident, from these facts, that, although the animal was living, its power of adaptation to marked changes in the external or internal environment was greatly less- ened, and this fact illustrates well the great general importance of the spinal cord and brain as reflex centers controlling the nutri- tion and co-ordinated activities of the body tissues and organs. This control is necessary under normal conditions for the success- ful combination of the activities of the various organs. A large part of this control is doubtless dependent upon the regulation of the blood supply to the various organs. The mechanism by which this is effected and the parts played by the cord and the brain (medulla oblongata), respectively, will be described in the section on circulation. Knee-jerk. — Knee-jerk or knee-kick is the name commonly given to the jerk of the foot when a light blow is struck upon the patellar ligament just below the knee. The jerk of the foot is due to a contraction of the quadriceps femoris muscle. According to Sherrington, the parts of this muscular mass chiefly concerned are the m. vastus medialis and m. femoralis. In order to obtain the muscular response it is usually necessary to put the quadriceps under some tension by flexion of the leg. This end is obtained most readily by crossing the knees or by allowing the leg to hang freely when sitting on the edge of a bench or table. Under such circumstances the jerk is obtained in the great majority of normal persons, and this fact has made it an important diagnostic sign in many diseases of the spinal cord. The importance of the reaction for such purposes was first brought out by the work of Erb and of Westphal* in 1875. Reinforcement of the Knee-jerk. — It was first shown by Jendrassik (1883) that the extent of the jerk may be greatly aug- mented if, at the time the blow is struck upon the tendon, a strong voluntary movement is made by the individual, such as squeezing the hands together tightly or clenching the jaws. This phenomenon was studied carefully in this country by Mitchell and Lewis,f who ascertained that a similar augmentation may be produced by giving the individual a simultaneous sensory stimulation. They desig- nated the phenomenon as a reinforcement, and this name is gen- erally employed by English writers, although occasionally the term "Bahnung," introduced by Exner to describe a similar phenom- enon, is also used. It is found that by a reinforcement the knee- * Erb and Westphal, "Archiv f. Psychiatrie," 1875, vol. v. t Mitchell and Lewis, " American Journal of Med. Sciences," 92, 363, 1886. REFLEX ACTIONS. 147 jerk may be demonstrated in some individuals in whom the ordi- nary blow upon the tendon fails to elicit a response. Bowditch and Warren* studied the phenomenon of reinforcement and brought out a fact of very great interest. They studied especially the time interval between the blow upon the tendon and the reinforcing act and found that if the latter preceded the blow by too great an inter- val then, instead of an augmentation of the jerk, there was a dimi- nution which they designated as negative reinforcement or inhi- bition. This inhibiting effect began to appear when the reinforcing act (hand-squeeze) preceded the blow by an interval of from 0.22 to 0.6 sec, and the maximum inhibiting effect was obtained at an mm Fig. 65. — Showing in millimeters the amount by which the "reinforced" knee-kick varied from the normal, the level of which is represented by the horizontal line at 0, "nor- mal." The time intervals elapsing between the clenching of the hand (which constituted the reinforcement) and the tap on the tendon are marked below. The reinforcement is greatest when the two events are nearly simultaneous. At an interval of 0.4 sec. it amounts to nothing: during the next 0.6 sec. the height of the kick is actually dimin- ished the longer the interval, after which the negative reinforcement tends to disappear; and when 1.7 sec. is allowed to elapse the height of the kick ceases to be affected by the clenching of the hand. — {Bowditch and Warren.) interval of from 0.6 to 0.9 sec. Beyond this point the effect became less noticeable, and at an interval of 1.7 to 2.5 sec. the reinforcing act had no influence at all upon the jerk. These relations are shown in the accompanying curve (Fig. 65). These authors con- firmed also the fact that a sensory stimulus, such as a gentle blast of air on the conjunctiva or the knee, may reinforce the jerk. The physiological explanation of the reinforcement, negative and posi- tive, is a matter of inference only, but the view usually held is that it is due to "overflow." That is, many facts, such as strychnin tetanus, indicate that the neuromuscular machinery of the entire central nervous system is more or less directly connected and that * Bowditch and Warren. "Journal of Physiology," 2, 25, 1890. 148 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. functional activity at one part will influence the irritability of the remainder either in the direction of reinforcement (Bahnung) or inhibition. We may conceive, therefore, that when the hands are squeezed the motor impulses sent down from the cortex of the brain to the upper portion of the cord overflow to some extent, sufficient at least to alter 'the irritability of the other motor neurons in the cord. Experimental stimulation of the cortex has given similar results. Exner* found that when the motor center for the foot in the cortex of a rabbit was stimulated, the stimulation, even if too weak to be effective itself, caused an increase in the contraction brought about reflexly by a simultaneous stimulation of the skin of the paw, and furthermore if these stimuli were so reduced in strength that each was ineffective, then when applied together a contraction was obtained. In this case an ineffective stimulus from the cortex reaching the spinal cord increased the irritability of the motor centers there so that a simultaneous reflex stimulus from the foot, ineffective in itself, became effective. Is the Knee-jerk a Reflex? — The most interesting question in this connection is whether the jerk is a true reflex act or is due to a direct mechanical stimulation of the muscle. Opinions are divided upon this point. Those who believe that the jerk is a reflex lay emphasis upon the undoubted fact that the integrity of the reflex arc is absolutely essential to the response. The quad- riceps receives its motor and sensory fibers through the anterior crural nerve, and pathological lesions upon man as well as direct experimental investigation upon monkeys prove that if either the posterior or anterior roots of the third and fourth lumbar spinal nerves are destroyed the knee-jerk disappears entirely. The oppo- nents of the reflex view explain this fact by the theory that in order for the quadriceps to respond it must be in a condition of tonus. This tonus depends upon the reflex arc, the sensory impulses from the muscle serving to keep it in that condition of subdued contraction known as tone. On this view destruc- tion of the reflex arc renders the muscle less irritable, so that it will not respond by a contraction to the sudden mechanical exten- sion or pull caused by the blow on the tendon. The adherents of this view lay emphasis upon two facts: First, the knee-jerk is a simple contraction, and not a tetanus, and, generally speaking, the motor centers of the cord discharge a series of impulses when stimulated. Second, the time for the jerk — that is, the interval between the stimulus and the response — is too short for a reflex. The determination of this time has been attempted by many ob- servers for the purpose of deciding the controversy, but unfor- tunately the results have been lacking in uniformity, varying, in * Exner, " Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie," 27, 412, 1882. REFLEX ACTIONS. 149 man, from 0.025 to 0.073 sec. Moreover, we have no definite basis upon which to estimate what should be the time required if the act were a genuine reflex. For the act of winking in man Exner estimated a total time of 0.0578 sec, but May hew obtained a smaller figure — 0.0471 sec. In the lower animals the results have also been uncertain. Applegarth, making use of a dog with a severed spinal cord, obtained for the time of the knee-jerk an interval of 0.014 to 0.02 sec. Assuming a velocity of 100 ft. or more per second, this would allow sufficient time for the impulse to travel to the cord and back provided there was no delay in the nerve centers. Waller and Gotch, using the rabbit, found the time to be only 0.008 to 0.005 sec, — that is, just about the latent period of a muscle contraction and too short a time' for a reflex. It is evident that more facts are necessary before a positive statement can be made upon this point. In favor of the reflex theory attention may be called to the fact that in some cases a crossed reflex is obtained affecting the muscles of the other leg. This apparently undoubted reflex shows that an efficient sensory impulse has reached the cord, and, according to our knowledge of reflexes, the effect in such cases should always be most marked on its own side. It would seem to be unjustifiable in these cases to suppose that the effect on the same side is not reflex while on the opposite side it is reflex. 'Conditions Influencing the Extent of the Knee-jerk. — The effect of varying normal conditions upon the knee-jerk has been studied by a number of observers, particularly by Lombard.* The results are most interesting in that they indicate very clearly that the irritability of the spinal cord varies with almost every marked variation in mental activity. During sleep the jerk disappears and in mental conditions of a restful character its extent is relatively small. In conditions of mental excitement or irritation, on the contrary, the jerk becomes markedly increased. Lombard ob- served also, in his own case, a daily rhythm, which is represented in the chart given in Fig. 66. It would seem from his experiments that the extent of the knee-jerk is a sensitive indicator of the relative state of irritability of the nervous system: "The knee- jerk is increased and diminished by whatever increases and di- minishes the activity of the central nervous system as a whole." This general fact is supported, especially as regards mental activity, by observations on other similar mechanisms, — such, for instance, as the condition of the nervous centers controlling the bladder. ♦Lombard, "The American Journal of Psychology," 1887, p. 1. See also article "Knee-jerk" (Warren), "Wood's Ref. Handbook of Med. Sci- ences," second edition, 1902. PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. Use of the JLnee-jerk and Spinal Reflexes as Diagnostic fQ$l [jSign^^T^jEeffct that the knee-jerk depends on the integrity of the fpftorgrc in thelumbar cord has made it useful as a diagnostic indication in lesions of the cord, particularly, of course, for the lumbar region. It is mainly on account of its practical value and the ease with which it is ordinarily obtained that the phenom- enon has been studied so extensively. In the disease known as progressive locomotor ataxia the posterior root fibers in the pos- terior columns in the lumbar region are affected, and, as a con- sequence, the jerk is diminished or abolished altogether according to the stage of the disease. So also lesions affecting the anterior • i % 66.— Lombard's figure to indicate the daily rhythm in the extent of the knee- jerk and the effect of mental stimuli. The ordinates (0-110) represent the extent of the kick m millimeters. Each dot represents a separate kick, while the heavy horizontal line gives the average extent for the period indicated. horns of the gray matter will destroy the reflex by cutting off the motor path, while in other cases lesions in the brain or the lateral columns of the cord affecting the pyramidal system of fibers may be accompanied by an exaggeration of this and similar reflexes. This latter fact agrees with the experimental results (see p. 140) upon ablation of the brain. After such operations in the frog and lower mammals at least the spinal reflexes may show a marked increase. Interruption of the descending connections between brain and cord at any point, therefore, may be accompanied by a strik- ing increase in sensitiveness of the spinal reflexes. The explana- tion usually given is that the inhibitory influences of the brain centers upon the cord are thereby weakened or destroyed. The baa dfnd a&muri ,'inmjsia«i(J — :.T8 .si'*l rob alb/bom ,2 ;aii«ohr»o bHuI>9M ,f :*nobj9n no] -ti ,,tni bofai nm^ox luiiiqs yrlJ uirl« 1« «bvoI 9dl .oaaswtoi lo moil 9di no qi* woria ot bna tn9vis 9-te ftgfosnm to aqumg no eoloawm k> as bie bnad-Jii 9dl dofdw ni bioa adt io ^tnsnfya* 9dt moil Vn ■ ill on.t I' A »> ' A <* V' S " r m 9 , » v 4 * • * * ■»«S«Vy*- Fig. 80. — Sections through corresponding parts of the cortex in: a, Man; &, dog; and c, mole, to show the greater separation of the nerve cells in the higher animals. — ■ (Bethe, after Nissl.) is increased. This anatomical fact would indicate that the greater mental activity in the higher animals is dependent, in part, upon the richer interconnection of the nerve cells, or, expressed physio- logically, our mental processes are characterized by their more numerous and complex associations. A visual or auditory stim- ulus that, in the frog, for instance, may call forth a comparatively simple motor response, may in man, on account of the numerous associations with the memory records of past experiences, lead to 12 178 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. psychical and motor responses of a much more intricate and in- direct character. Extirpation of the Cerebrum. — One of the methods used in physiology to determine the general functional value of the cerebral hemispheres has been to remove them completely, by surgical operation, and to study the effect upon the psychical responses of the animal. Upon the cold-blooded animals and the birds the operation may be performed with ease, but in these animals the positive results are not striking and the experiments are valuable chiefly for their negative results. If the cerebral hemispheres are removed from the frog, for example, the animal after recovering from the immediate effects of the operation — that is, the effects of the anesthetic and the shock — shows surprisingly little difference from the normal animal. It maintains a normal posture and shows no loss at all in its power of equilibration. When placed on its back it quickly regains its usual position. If thrown into water it swims to a solid support and crawls out like a normal animal. It jumps when stimulated and is careful to avoid obstacles placed in its way, showing that its visual reflexes are not impaired. It is said, however, that the more complicated reactions that depend upon the memory of past experien ces or the instincts are absent or imperfect. This latter peculiarity is manifested most impressively in birds (pigeons) after removal of a part or all of the cerebrum. As a result of such an operation, the nervous, active animal is changed at once to a stupid, lethargic creature which reacts only when stimulated. It sits in a drowsy attitude, with its head drawn in to the shoulders, its eyes closed, and its feathers slightly erected; occasionally it will open its eyes, stretch the neck, gape, preen its feathers perhaps, and then sink back into its somnolent attitude. The animal in this condition maintains its equilibrium perfectly, flies well if thrown into the air and perches comfortably upon a narrow support. It may be kept alive apparently indefinitely by appropriate feeding and so long as it is well fed retains its stupid and impassive appearance. If allowed to starve for a while it becomes restless from the effects of hunger, may walk to and fro, and peck aimlessly at the ground. If surrounded by grain it may peck at the separate grains, but never actually seizes one in its beak and swallows it. The striking defect in these animals is the loss of those responses that depend upon memory of past or in- herited experiences. Its motor reactions are all of a simple kind. If placed upon a hot plate it will, for a time, lift first one foot, then the other, and finally squat, but never flies away. When dosing a loud noise awakens it, but it exhibits no signs of fear, and quickly relapses into somnolence when the auditory stimulus ceases. The one positive conclusion that we may draw from the behavior GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF THE CEREBRUM. 179 of these animals is that in them the cerebrum is the place in which the memory records are stored, and that when it is removed the actions of the animal when stimulated become much more direct and predictable, since the stimulus awakens no associations with past experiences. The complete removal of the cerebrum in mam- mals is attended with more difficulty. When taken out at once by a single operation the animal survives but a short time and the permanent effects of the operation cannot be detected. Goltz,* however, has succeeded, in dogs, in removing by a peculiar operation all of the cerebral cortex. The operation was performed in sev- eral successive stages with an interval of several months between. In the most successful experiment the animal was kept alive for a year and a half and the postmortem examination showed that all of the cortex had been removed except a small portion of the tip of the temporal lobe, and this latter, since its connections with the other parts of the brain had been destroyed, was, of course, functionless. In addition, a large part of the corpora striata and optic thalami and a small portion of the midbrain had been re- moved. The behavior of this animal was studied carefully. After the immediate effects of the operation — paralysis, etc. — had disap- peared the animal moved easily ; in fact, showed a tendency to keep moving continually. There was no permanent paralysis of the so- called voluntary movements. He answered to sensory stimuli of various kinds, but not in an intelligent way. If, for instance, a painful stimulus was applied to the skin, he would growl or bark, and turn his head toward the place stimulated; but did not attempt to bite. No caressing could arouse signs of pleasure, and no threatening signs of fear or anger. Like the pigeon, the most con- spicuous defect in the animal was a lack of intelligent response, — that is, the responses to sensory stimuli were simple, and evidently did not involve complex associations with past experiences. His memory records, for the most part, had been destroyed. Goltz records that when starved he showed signs of hunger, and that eventually he learned to feed himself when his nose was brought into contact with the food, although he was not able to recognize food placed near him. He would reject food with a disagreeable taste. When sleeping he gave no signs of dreaming, differing in this respect from normal dogs. Localization of Functions in the Cerebrum. — When the belief was established that the cerebrum is the organ of the higher psychical activities there arose naturally the question whether dif- ferent parts of the cortex have different functions corresponding to the various faculties of the mind, or whether the cerebrum is functionally equivalent throughout, in the same sense, for instance, * Goltz, "Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie," 51, 570, 1892. 180 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. as the liver. This question of the localization of functions in the brain (cerebrum) has been much debated, but the most interesting and important discussions upon the subject belong to the nine- teenth century. About the beginning of the century Franz Joseph Gall, at that time a physician in Vienna, began to teach publicly his well-known system of cranioscopy or, as it was later designated by his chief disciple (Spurzheim) , system of phrenology.* Gall, from his early youth, was possessed with the idea that the different faculties of the mind are mediated through different parts of the brain, that in it we have to deal not with a single, but with a plurality of organs. This belief was in opposition to the current ideas of his times and Gall devoted his entire life to an earnest effort to estab- lish and popularize his views. He and his disciples contributed many very important facts to our knowledge of the finer anatomy of the brain; but, so far as the view of separate organs in the cerebrum is concerned, the methods that he employed, although perhaps the only ones that he could make use of at that time, have since been demonstrated to be fallacious when used as he used them. He conceived that the more developed any given mental quality is the larger will be the organ representing it in the cerebrum, and since the cranium fits closely to the cerebrum the relative prominence of the parts of the cerebrum may be judged by a study of the exterior of the skull. This method of study con- stituted the essential feature of cranioscopy or phrenology, and by observation upon people with particularly marked mental qualities Gall and his disciples supposed that they had located the organs for thirty-five different faculties. While the general idea of this method may be defended, it is obvious that the application of it scientifically, so that positive and demonstrable results can be obtained, is practically impossible. The system of phrenology and its methods quickly fell into disrepute, particularly as it soon became a favorite implement for the use of frauds and charlatans. Gall's ideas in the beginning excited the greatest interest, but it seems that he was never able to convince the majority of the scien- tific men of his day of the conclusiveness of his results. At the time that he was exploiting his doctrines in Paris, where he spent the latter years of his life, Flourens began his celebrated experi- mental work upon the functions of the brain, — work which was mainly instrumental in convincing physiologists that the cerebrum is a single organ, functionally equivalent in all of its parts. f Flou- rens's chief experiments were made upon pigeons, and in these * Gall (and Spurzheim), "Recherches sur la systeme nerveux en general et sur celui du cerveau en particulier," 1810-1819. t Flourens, "Recherches experimentales sur les proprieties et les fonctions du systeme nerveux dans les animaux vertebres," 1824. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF THE CEREBRUM. 181 animals he found that successive ablations of parts of the cerebrum from before backward or from side to side was not followed by a corresponding series of defects in the animals' psychical life. On the contrary, when the quantity of brain substance removed was sufficiently large all these qualities went at once. The choice of animals for these experiments was an unfortunate one, but the results were corroborated in part by a number of instances in which human beings by acci- dent or wounds in battle had lost a part of the brain with- out any apparent defect in their mental powers. There- fore toward the middle of the nineteenth century the preva- lent view in physiology was that the cerebrum is function- ally equivalent in all of its parts. One fact was known in medicine at that time which distinctly contradicted this be- lief , — namely, that an injury to the posterior portion of the third frontal convolution in man, on the left side, causes a loss of articulate speech (motor aphasia). But this fact, so sig- nificant to us now, was not properly valued at the time. The beginning of our modern views of cerebral localization is found in the work of Fritsch and Hitzig* (1870), in which they exposed and stimulated electrically the cortex cerebri in dogs. They found that stimu- lation of certain definite areas, particularly in the sigmoid gyrus, gave distinct and constant movements in the limbs, face, etc. (see Fig. 81). This work was followed quickly by experiments of a similar kind made by numerous observers, in which the cerebrum was stimulated in various animals and finally in man. In addition, the method of ablation of these areas was employed with subsequent study of the animal in regard to the motor or sensory defects result- * Fritsch and Hitzig, "Archiv f. Anatomie und Physiologie und wissen- schaftliche Medizin," 1870, 300. Fig. 81. — To show the motor areas in the dog's brain as originally determined by Fritsch and Hitzig: s, Sigmoid gyrus; A, center for the neck muscles; -f , center for the ex- tensors and adductors of the forelimb; +, center for the flexors and rotation of fore- limb; #, center for the hind limb; O — O, center for the muscles innervated by the facial. 182 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. ing therefrom, and the results obtained were further extended by careful autopsies upon human beings in whom paralyses of various kinds and sensory defects were associated with more or less definite lesions of the cerebrum. The first outcome of this work was to lead to an extreme view of localization of function in the brain, in which the different motor and sensory areas were definitely circumscribed and separated one from the other, making the cere- brum a plurality of organs, to use Gall's term. The more recent Fig. 82. — Diagram representing the probable location of the chief motor and sensory areas of the cerebral cortex in man: A, Lateral surface ; B, mesial surface. — (From Schafer.) work has tended to modify these extreme views of localization and to emphasize the fact that histologically and physiologically the entire cerebrum is connected so intimately, part to part, that, although the different regions mediate different functions, never- theless an injury or defect in one part may influence to some extent the functional value of all other regions in the organ. The general idea of a localization of function has been established definitely, but the modern view is that the cerebrum is composed of a plurality GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF THE CEREBRUM. 183 of organs, not completely separated one from the other as taught by Gall, but intimately associated and to a certain extent dependent one on another for their full functional importance. The Motor Area. — The first experiments of Fritsch and Hitzig disclosed the location of a cortical region in the dog which upon stimulation gave definite movements. The later experiments of Ferrier, Schafer, Horsley, and Beevor, particularly upon the apes, gave reason for believing that this motor area surrounds the fissure, of Rolando and extends inward upon the mesial surface of the cere- brum. Its exact boundaries marked out by careful stimulation of Toes „Ankle \ Artec Wrist. Tinkers Abdomen-. jQvest Eyelid /Closure Sulcus Centralis Nose °fjal° Oberiing Vaca,} Mastication, ofjau> cords Fig. 83. — Location of motor areas in brain of chimpanzee. — {Sherrington and Green- baum.) The extent of the motor areas is indicated by stippling; it lies entirely in front of the fissure of Rolando (sulcus centralis). Much of the motor area is hidden in the sulci. The regions marked eyes indicate the areas whose stimulation gives conjugate movements of the eyeballs. It is doubtful, however, whether these represent motor areas proper. the region in monkeys was more or less verified upon man, since in operations upon the brain it was often necessary to stimulate the cor- tex in order to localize a given motor area. By these means charts have been made showing the cortical area for the musculature of each part of the body. The location of these areas as usually given is represented, for the human brain, by Fig. 82. It will be seen that in general the distribution of the areas along the fissure of Rolando follows the order of the cranial and spinal nerves. Within each area smaller centers may be located by careful stimulation; thus, the hand and arm area may be subdivided into centers for the wrist, fingers, thumb, etc. More recently Sherrington and Green- 184 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. baum,* making use of electrical stimulation, unipolar method, have explored carefully the motor areas in the monkey. They state that these areas do not extend back of the Rolandic fissure, but lie chiefly along the anterior central convolution, as represented in Fig. 83, extending for only a small distance on to the mesial surface of the cerebrum. The area thus delimited by physiological experiments is the region from which arises the pyramidal system of fibers, and clinical experience has shown that lesions in this part of the cortex are accompanied by a paralysis of the muscles on the other side, particu- larly in the limbs. Pathological or experimental lesions here, moreover, are followed by a degeneration of the pyramidal neurons, — a degeneration which extends to the termination of the neuron in the cord. With these data we can construct a fairly com- plete account of the mechanism of voluntary movements. The initial impulse arises in the large pyramidal cells of the motor areas and proceeds along the axons of their neurons to the motor nuclei of the cranial or spinal nerves. The neurons of the pyramidal tract constitute the motor tract for voluntary move- ments; a lesion anywhere along this tract causes paralysis, more or less complete and on the other side of the body in general, if the le- sion is anterior to the decussation. The path of the motor fibers is represented in the schema given in Fig. 84. Arising in the cortex, they take the following route (see also Fig. 77,5): 1. Corona radiata. 2. Internal capsule. 3. Cms cerebri (pes). 4. Pons Varolii, in which they are broken into a number of smaller bundles by the fibers of the middle peduncle of the * "Reports of the Thompson- Yates and Johnston Laboratories," 4, 351, 1902; 5, 55, 1903. Fig. 84. — Schema representing the course of the fibers of the pyram- idal tract: 1, Fibers to the nuclei of the cranial nerve; 2, uncrossed fibers to the lateral pyramidal tract; 3, fi- bers to the anterior pyramidal tract crossing in the cord; 4 and 5, fibers that cross in the pyramidal decussa- tion to make the lateral pyramidal tract of the opposite side. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF THE CEREBRUM. 185 cerebellum. In this region, also, some of the fibers cross the mid-line, to end in the motor nuclei of the cranial nerves: Third, fourth, fifth, sixth, and seventh. 5. Anterior pyramids. 6. Pyramidal decussation. 7. Direct and crossed pyramidal tracts in the cord. After ending in the motor nuclei of the cranial or spinal nerves the path is continued by a second neuron from these nuclei to the mus- cles. The entire path involves, therefore, two neurons, and injury to either will cause paralysis of the corresponding muscles. Difference in the Paralysis from Injury to the Spinal and the Pyramidal Neuron. — With regard to the musculature of the limbs especially a difference has been observed in the paralysis caused by injury to the spinal and pyramidal neurons, respectively. Lesions of the anterior root cells in the cord or of the axons arising from them cause complete paralysis of the corresponding muscles, since these muscles are then removed not only from voluntary control, but also from reflex effects. The muscles are entirely relaxed and in time ex- hibit a more or less complete atrophy. When the pyramidal neurons alone are affected, as in the familiar condition of hemiplegia resulting from a unilateral lesion of the motor cortex, there is paralysis as regards voluntary control, but, the spinal neuron being intact, the muscles are still subject to reflex stimulation through the cord, especially to the so-called tonic impulses. Under these conditions it is frequently noticed that the paralyzed muscles are thrown into a state of continuous contraction, contracture, in which they exhibit a spastic rigidity. This fact, therefore, may be used in diagnosing the general location of the lesion. Is the Pyramidal Tract the Only Means of Voluntary (Cortical) Control of the Muscles? — Much discussion has arisen regarding this question. It is, in fact, one of those questions of nervous mechanism in which experiments upon lower ani- mals must be applied with caution to the conditions in man. As we have seen, the entire cerebral cortex may be removed from the frog, the pigeon, and the dog without causing permanent paralysis, although in the animal last named there is at first a more or less marked loss of voluntary control. But in man and the higher types of the monkey the pyramidal system is more completely developed, and corresponding with this fact it is found that the paralysis from lesion of the motor cortex is more permanent. In fact, observations upon men in whom it has been necessary to re- move parts of the motor area by surgical operation indicate that the voluntary control of the muscles is lost or impaired permanently. It would seem, therefore, that in an animal as high in the scale as the dog voluntary control of the muscles can be maintained through 186 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. tracts other than the pyramidal system, tracts, perhaps, such as Monakow's bundle (rubrospinal tract), arising in the midbrain. In man, however, along with the more complete development of the pyramidal system, the efficacy of the phylogenetically older motor systems is correspondingly reduced. The Crossed Control of the Muscles and Bilateral Repre- sentation in the Cortex. — It has been known from very ancient times that an injury to the brain on one side is accompanied by a paralysis of voluntary movement on the other side of the body, a condition known as hemiplegia. The facts given above regarding the origin and course of the pyramidal fibers explain the crossed character of the paralysis quite satisfactorily. The schema thus presented to us is, however, not entirely without exception. In cases of hemiplegia in which the whole motor area is involved it is known that the paralysis on the other side does not involve all the muscles, and, in the second place, it is said that there is some mus- cular weakness on the same side. The paralysis in hemiplegia affects but little, if at all, those muscles of the trunk which are accus- tomed to act in unison, — the muscles of inspiration, for instance, the diaphragm, abdominal and intercostal muscles, and the muscles of the larynx. It would appear that these muscles are bilaterally represented in the cortex; so that if one side of the brain is intact the muscles of both sides are still under voluntary control. The mechanism of this bilateral representation is not definitely known ; one might conceive several possibilities. The motor area on each side may send down a double set of pyramidal fibers, one of which crosses and the other remains on the same side, or the fibers may bifurcate. Or it is possible that the bilateral control is due to commissural connections between the lower centers in the cord. Some evidence in favor of the former view is found in the undoubted histological fact brought out by Melius and others, that small unilateral lesions in the motor area — the center of the great toe in the monkey, for instance — are followed by degeneration in the lateral pyramidal tract in the cord on both sides, showing that some portions of the motor area send fibers to both sides of the body. In cases of hemiplegia it may be added that the muscles of the limbs are not all equally affected. Are the Motor Areas Only Motor in Function? — The great number of nerve cells in the cortex in addition to the large pyram- idal cells that give origin to the fibers of the pyramidal tract make it possible histologically that other functions may be mediated in the same region. This possibility has been kept in view since the early experiments of Munk, in which he showed that lesions in the Rolandic region are followed by disturbances in what are designated as the body sensations, muscular and cutaneous sensi- GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF THE CEREBRUM. 187 bility, but especially the former. It was suggested, therefore, at one time that one and the same spot in the cortex might serve as the origin of the motor impulses to a given muscle and as the cortical termination of the sensory impulses coming from the same muscle, the reaction in consciousness, the muscular sensa- tions, being mediated perhaps through cells other than those giving rise to the pyramidal fibers. Recent physiological and clin- ical work has, however, not tended to support this view. The motor areas appear to be confined to the region in front of the fis- sure of Rolando, while the cortical area which gives rise to that kind of consciousness that we designate in general as body sensi- bility extends back of the Rolandic fissure in the posterior central gyrus. Whether, on the other hand, the sense areas for the body (cutaneous and muscular) extend forward into the cortex of the frontal lobe is not clearly shown by experimental or clinical evidence. Flechsig, from his studies upon the time of myelini- zation of the afferent fibers in the embryo brain, concludes that this is the case, and that therefore the motor and sensory areas overlap for a part at least of their extent (see p. 210 and Fig. 93). In the motor area there are numerous connections by afferent fibers, association tracts, with other parts of the brain. By this means the motor area, without doubt, is brought into rela- tion with many other parts of the cortex, and the sensations or perceptions aroused elsewhere may react upon the motor paths. A voluntary movement, however simple it may be, is a psychological act of some complexity. The mental processes that lead to and originate the motor discharge cannot be located in the cortex, but the immediate origin of the motor impulse lies most probably, in the areas along the anterior margin of the fissure of Rolando. CHAPTER X. THE SENSE AREAS AND THE ASSOCIATION AREAS OF THE CORTEX. The delimitation of the sensory areas in the cortex is a matter of very considerable difficulty, owing, mainly, to the fact that the determination of the presence or absence of certain states of con- sciousness in the animal or person under observation cannot be made except by indirect . means. Moreover, the distinction be- tween what we may call simple sensations and the more complex psychical representations and judgments of which these sensations form a necessary constituent can not be made clearly, even by the individual in whom the reactions occur. We recognize in ourselves different stages in the degree of consciousness aroused by sensory reactions. Our visual and auditory sensations are clearly dif- ferentiated ; but many of the lower senses escape recognition in the individual himself, since the state of consciousness accom- panying them is of a lower order. Our muscular sensations, for in- stance, are so indefinite as to be practically subconscious. They are most important to us in every act of our lives, yet the unin- formed person is unconscious of the existence of such a sensation, and if deprived of it would recognize the defect only in the con- sequent loss of control of the voluntary muscular movements. In the attempts to determine in what part of the brain the various sensations are mediated every possible method of inquiry has been used : the anatomical course of the sensory paths, physiological ex- periments of stimulation and ablation, and observations upon in- dividuals with pathological or traumatic lesions in the brain. In the long run, the study of neuropathological cases in man must give us the last word, because in such cases the estimate of the sensory defect can be made with most accuracy and because in man the specialization of the psychical functions has reached its highest development. The results that have been obtained are perhaps the most definite in the case of the higher senses, vision and hearing, since defects in these senses are recognized most clearly, and the anatomical mechanisms involved have proved to be more accessi- ble to investigation. The Body-sense Area. — In his early experiments Munk in- sisted that lesions of the cortex involving the Rolandic area are 188 SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 189 accompanied by a state of anesthesia on the other side of the body, hemianesthesia, particularly as regards the tactile and muscular sensations. It is not necessary, perhaps, to go into the details of the long controversy that arose in connection with this point. Both the clinical and the experimental evidence has been contra- dictory in the hands of different observers, but the tendency of recent studies has been to show, as stated above, that, whereas the motor areas lie anterior to the fissure of Rolando, the sensory areas concerned with the cutaneous and muscular sensations extend posterior to this fissure.* Positive cases are recorded in which lesions involving the anterior central convolutions were accompanied by paralysis on the other side, hemiplegia, without any detectable disturbance of sensibility, and, on the other hand, lesions in the posterior central, and neighboring parietal convolutions, in which there was a hemianesthesia more or less distinctly marked without any paralysis. Such cases tend to support the view that the motor and body sense areas, although contiguous, do not overlap.f On the other hand, the embryological evidence, as furnished by Flechsig, indicates that the sense areas may extend in front of the Rolandic fissure (p. 210) and overlap the motor areas in part. At present, perhaps, one is justified in saying only that the region immediately posterior to the Rolandic fissure is entirely sensory. Regarding the sensory defects associated with lesions of the parietal lobe posterior to the Rolandic fissure (posterior central convolution, supramarginal, superior, and possibly inferior parietal convolutions), it seems probable that they involve chiefly the muscular sense, pressure and temperature sense, and the judgments or perceptions based upon these sensations, while the sense of pain is but little affected. Monakow gives the order in which sensory defects mani- fest themselves after such lesions, as follows: The localizing space and muscle sense are chiefly affected, —in fact, almost lost on the opposite side; the temperature and pressure sense are largely affected, while the pain sense is retained or but slightly affected. The clinicians have observed that the most positive and invariable symptom of lesions in this region is a condition of astereognosis, — that is, a diminution in the stereognostic sense or feeling. By the stereognostic feeling is meant the power to judge concerning the form and consistency of external objects when handled, and it must be regarded as a perception based upon the sensations of touch and temperature in combination with muscular sensibility. On the whole, therefore, we must infer that the cortex in this postrolandic area is concerned with the finer and more conscious * Consult Monakow, " Ergebnisse der Physiologie," 1902, vol. i, part I, p. 621. t Mills, American Neurological Association, 1901. 190 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. interpretations of the sensations of pressure, temperature, and muscular conditions. The part of the cortex, if any, concerned in the primary conscious mediation of the sense of pain has not been definitely localized. The Histological Evidence. Course of the " Fillet. "— On the histological side there is very strong corroborative evidence for the view that the cortical centers for the sensory fibers of the body Fissure of Rolando Internal or median 1111617 s-InternalJlrcifornv T^bres ofhinal CorcL Fig. 85. — Schema representing the origin and course of the fibers of the median fillet, — the intercentral paths of the fibers of body sense. in general lie in the parietal lobe in the region indicated above. This evidence is connected with the path taken by the sensory fibers in the cord, especially those of the posterior columns, after ending in the nucleus gracilis and nucleus cuneatus of the medulla. This path is represented in a schematic way in the accompanying diagram (Fig. 85). The second sensory neurons arise in the nuclei mentioned. Some of them pass into the cerebellum by way of the SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 191 inferior peduncle of the same side; but others, passing ventrally, cross the mid-line as the internal arciform fibers, which form a conspicuous feature in the tegmental region of sections of the medulla at this level. This crossing occurs mainly just in front of — that is, cephalad to — the pyramidal decussation, forming thus a sensory decussation, which explains the crossed sensory control, as the pyramidal decussation explains the crossed motor control of the cerebrum in relation to the body. After this decussation the sensory fibers form a longitudinal bundle on each side known as the median fillet, lemniscus, or laqueus, which in the pons lies just dorsal to the pyramidal fibers. The fillet fibers may be traced forward as far as the anterior collie- Fig. 86. — Cross-section through midbrain (Kolliker) to show the position of the fillet (L, L): Nr, The red nucleus ; Sn, the substantia nigra ; Pp, the crus. ulus of the corpora quadrigemina and the thalamus (see Fig. 86), and some are said to continue directly into the cerebrum by way of the posterior limb of the internal capsule to end in the parietal lobe posterior to the fissure of Rolando. Those neurons that end in the midbrain and thalamus are continued forward by a third neuron, which ends in the parietal lobe in the same region (see Fig. 77, C) . On its way through the medulla and pons the fillet tract is believed to receive accessions of sensory fibers from the sensory nuclei of the cranial nerves of the opposite side. The course of the fillet has been traced by various means, but especially by the method of myeliniza- tion during embryonic life and by degeneration consequent upon long-standing disuse. As was stated in the section upon nerve de- 192 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. generation, injury to an axon is followed quickly by degeneration of the peripheral end, and much more slowly by a degeneration of the central end and the nerve cell itself, when the path is not again estab- lished. Certain long-standing cystic lesions (porencephaly) in the parietal cortex have resulted in an atrophic degeneration of the fillet fibers, thus adding materially to the evidence that this sensory tract ends eventually in the region indicated,* From the connections of the fillet with the tracts of the pos- terior columns of the cord it is evident that it forms one pathway at least for the fibers of muscle sense. Whether or not the fibers of pressure, pain, and temperature take the same route is not definitely established, but it seems probable at least from the known connections of the fillet with the sensory nuclei of the cranial nerves and with the sensory tracts of the lateral as well as the posterior columns of the cord. Much of the fillet ends in the mid- brain and thalamus, before passing on to the cortex, and here, as in other similar cases, we have the possibility that the lower centers, in addition to the reflex connections which they make, may mediate also a conscious reaction. While the general tendency has been to confine the conscious quality of the central reactions to the cortex, there is no proof that the lower centers are entirely lacking in this property. In Goltz's dog without cerebral cortex, for instance, the animal responded to various sensory stimuli, and when hungry gave evidence, so far as his actions were concerned, of experiencing the sensations of hunger; but whether or not these actions were associated with conscious sensations is hidden from us, and we can hope to arrive at positive conclusions upon this point only by observations upon man himself. The Center for Vision. — The location in the cortex of the general area for vision has been established by anatomical, physio- logical, and clinical evidence. The physiologists have experimented chiefly by the method of ablation. Munk, Ferrier, and later ob- servers have found that removal of both occipital lobes is followed by defects in vision. According to Munk, removal of both occip- ital lobes is followed by complete loss of visual sensations, or, as he expresses it, by cortical blindness. Goltz, however, contends that in the dog at least removal of the entire cerebral cortex leaves the animal with some degree of vision, since he will close his eyes if a strong light is thrown upon them. All the experiments upon the higher mammals (monkeys) and clinical experience upon man tends, however, to support the view of Munk. Complete removal of the occipital lobes is followed by apparently total blindness. If any degree of vision remains it is not sufficient for recogni- tion of familiar objects or for directing the movements. In an * Hosel, "Archiv f. Psychiatrie," 24, 452, 1892. SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 193 animal in this condition the pupil becomes constricted when light is thrown upon the eye; but this reaction we may regard as a reflex through the midbrain, and there is no reason to believe that it is accompanied by a visual sensation. When the injury to the occip- ital cortex is unilateral the blindness affects symmetrical halves of the two eyes, a condition known as hemiopia. Destruction of the right occipital lobe causes blindness in the two right halves of the eyes, or in accordance with the law of projection of retinal stimuli in the two left halves of the normal visual field when the eyes are fixed upon any object. Destruction of the left oc- cipital lobe is followed by blindness in the two left halves of the retinas or the right halves of the visual field. This result of physiological experiments is borne out by clinical experience. Any unilateral injury to the occipital lobes is followed by a con- dition of hemiopia more or less complete according to the extent of the lesion. Observation, however, has shown that this general symmetrical -relation has one interesting and peculiar exception. The most important part of the retina in vision is the region of the foveie centrales, whose projection into the visual field constitutes the field of direct or central vision. It is said that the hemiopia from unilateral lesions of the cortex does not involve this part of the retina. The Histological Evidence. — The histological results supple- ment in a very satisfactory way the findings from physiology and pathology. The retina itself, considered from an embryological standpoint, is an outgrowth from the brain vesicles, and is there- fore an outlying portion of the central nervous system. The optic fibers, in terms of the neuron doctrine, must be considered as axons of the nerve cells in the retina. If, therefore, an eye is enu- cleated or an optic nerve is cut the fibers connected with the brain undergo secondary degeneration and their course can be traced microscopically to the brain. By this means it has been shown that in man and the mammalia there is a partial decus- sation of the optic fibers in the chiasma. The fibers from the inner side of each retina cross at this point to the opposite optic tract; those from the outer side of the retina do not decussate, but pass into the optic tract of the same side. The fibers of the optic tract end mainly in the gray matter of the external genicu- late body, but some pass also to the optic thalamus (pulvinar) and some to the anterior colliculus of the corpora quadrigemina. These locations, therefore, particularly the external geniculates, must be considered as the primary optic centers. From these points the path is continued toward the cortex by new neurons whose axons consti- tute a special bundle, the optic radiation, lying in the posterior limb of the internal capsule (see Fig. 77, D). A schema representing 13 194 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. this course of the optic fibers is given in the accompanying diagram (Fig. 87). According to this schema, the general relations of each occipital lobe to the retinas of the two eyes is such that the right occipital cortex represents the cortical center for the two right halves of the retinas, while the left occipital lobe is the center for the two left halves of each retina, — a relation that agrees completely with the results of experimental physiology and clinical studies. In addition to the fibers described, which may be regarded as the Occipital lobe. Optic radiation. Superior colliculus. Lateral or external geniculate. Optic thalamus. Retina. Fig. 87. — Diagram to indicate the general course of the fibers of the optic nerves and the bilateral connection between cortex and retina. visual fibers proper, there are other fibers in the optic tracts and optic nerves whose physiological value is not entirely clear. The fibers of this kind that have been described are: (1) Posterior or Gudden's commissure. Fibers that pass from one optic tract to the other along the posterior border of the chiasma. These fibers form a commissural band connecting the two internal (or median) geniculate bodies, and possibly also the inferior colliculi. It seems probable that they belong to the central auditory path SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 195 rather than to the visual system. (2) Fibers passing from the chiasma into the floor of the third ventricle. The further course of these fibers is not clearly known, but it is possible that they make connections with the nuclei of the third nerve. They will be referred to in the section on vision in connection with the light reflex of the iris. (3) An anterior commissure. Several observers have claimed that there is a commissural band along the anterior margin of the chiasma which connects one optic nerve or retina with the other. In accordance with this claim it is said that when a local lesion is made experimentally in one retina the degeneration that extends backward along the corresponding optic nerve is contin- ued in part over into the optic nerve of the other side. The exist- ence of this interesting commissural connection between the retinas is, however, still a matter of much uncertainty. There are many points in connection with the course of the optic fibers and the physiology of the different parts of the occip- ital cortex which are unknown and require further investigation. Some of these points may be referred to briefly. The Amount of Decussation in the Chiasma. — According to the schema given above, half of the fibers in each optic nerve decussate in the chiasma. There is, however, no positive proof that the division of the fibers is so symmetrically made. In the lower vertebrates, — fishes, amphibia, reptiles, and most birds — the crossing is said to be complete, while in the mammalia a certain proportion of the fibers remain in the optic tract of the same side. In a general way, it would appear that the higher the animal is in the scale of development the larger is the number of fibers that do not cross in the chiasma. At least it is true that a larger num- ber remain uncrossed in man than in any of the mammalia, and it is also possible or probable that the extent of decussation in man shows individual differences. There seems to be no acceptable suggestion regarding the physiological value of this partial decus- sation other than that of a probable relation to binocular vision. It has been used to explain the physiological fact that simultaneous stimulation of symmetrical points in the two retinas gives us a single visual sensation. The Projection or Localization of the Retina on the Occipital Cortex. — It would seem most probable that the paths from each spot in the retina terminate in a definite region of the occipital cortex, and attempts have been made by various methods to determine this relation. According to Henschen,* the visual paths in man end around the calcarine fissure (Fig. 82) on the mesial surface of the brain, and this portion of the occipital lobe should be regarded as the true cortical center for vision, the re- * Henschen, " Brain," 1893, 170. 196 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. mainder of the occipital cortex being perhaps the seat of visual memories or associations. There seems to be much evidence indeed that the immediate ending of the optic paths lies in this region. Thus, Donaldson* found, upon examination of the brain of Laura Bridgman, — the blind deaf-mute, that the cuneus espe- cially showed marked atrophy, and clinical cases of lesions of the cuneus have been found to be associated with a marked degree of hemiopia. So also Flechsig,t by means of the myelinization method, finds that the optic fibers end chiefly along the margin of the calcarine fissure. It has been assumed that the fibers from the fovea of the retina LEFT RETINA RlGHTRETINA. end in this region, — according to some authors (Henschen) along the anterior third of the fissure, according to others (Schmid and LaqueurJ) along the posterior portion of the fissure. Moreover, since uni- lateral lesions of the occipital lobe, however extensive, do not cause complete blindness of the foveal region, it has been supposed that this im- portant part of the retina is bilaterally represented in the cortex, as indicated in the ac- companying diagram (Fig. 88) ; so that complete foveal blind- ness— that is, blindness of the centers of the visual fields — can only occur when both occipital lobes are injured in the region of the calcarine fissure. While the general opinion seems to be that this last-named region is the main cortical ending of the retinal fibers, especially of those arising from the foveal area, other observers contend that the entire occipital cortex, lateral as well as mesial surfaces, must be regarded as the cortical termination of the visual paths, and that even the foveal portion of the retina is con- nected with a wide area in this lobe. Monakow, for instance, points out that, while extensive lesions of the occipital cortex on both sides, leave, with a few exceptions, some degree of central vision, * Donaldson, "American Journal of Psychology," 1892, 4. f Flechsig, "Localization der geistigen Vorgange," Leipzig, 1896. j Schmid and Laqueur, "Virchow's Archiv," 158, 1900. Fig. 88. — Diagram showing the probable relations between the parts of the retina and the visual area of the cortex. — (From Schd- fer.) The bilateral representation of the fovea is indicated by the course of the dotted lines. SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 197 no cases are reported of cortical lesions involving only or mainly the vision in the macular region. He therefore argues that, while the paths from the retina to the lower visual centers (external geniculate) may be isolated, the further connections with the cortex must be widespread. The cortical center for distinct vision according to this view is not limited to a narrow area, but must involve a large region in the occipital cortex. It is difficult to reconcile this view with the ideas of isolated conduction and specific function of each part of the cortex, and it is very evident that the projection of the retinas upon the cortex is a question that must be left for further observation and experiment. Some light was supposed to be thrown upon the subject from the results of stimula- tion of the occipital cortex. Stimulation of this kind causes move- ments of the eyes, and the movements vary with the place stimu- lated.* Stimulation of the upper border of the lobe causes move- ments of the eyes downward, stimulation of the lower border move- ments upward and of intermediate regions movements to the side. Assuming that the direction of the movement is associated with movements toward that part of the visual field from which a normal visual stimulus would come, it is evident that movements of the eyes downward would imply stimulation of the upper half of the retina, since objects in the lower part of the visual field form their image on the upper half of the retina. Following this suggestion, the projection of the retinas on the occipital lobes, or the cortical representation of the retinas on the occipital cortex might be represented by a definite schema. Such a definite rela- tionship, however, as stated above, is not borne out by clinical facts. The fact that stimulation of the occipital cortex causes definite movements of the eyeballs seems, however, to be demonstrated and it implies that there are efferent fibers in the optic radiation running from the occipital cortex to the midbrain, where they make con- nections with the motor nuclei of the third, fourth, and sixth cranial nerves. The Function of the Lower Visual Centers.— The first ending of the optic fibers lies in the external geniculate and to a lesser extent in the thalamus and superior colliculus. It is con- ceivable, of course, that some degree of visual sensation may be mediated through these centers. Goltz observed that in dogs with the cerebrum removed the animals showed a constriction of the pupils when a bright light was thrown upon the eyes, or even closed the eyes. It is the general belief that reactions of this kind are me- chanical reflexes accompanied by no higher psychical reaction than in the case of spinal reflexes. The existence in the midbrain of the motor nuclei of the third nerve, and of the posterior longitudinal * Schafer, "Brain," 11, 1, 1889, and 13, 165, 1890. 198 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. bundle through which connections are established with the motor nuclei of other cranial nerves, furnishes us with a possible reflex arc through which the visual impulses brought into the lower optic centers may cause co-ordinated movements of the eyes or of the head. Usually it is assumed that conscious visual sensations, and especially visual associations and memories, are aroused only after the impulses reach the occipital cortex. In the fishes the midbrain forms the final ending of the optic fibers and in these animals, therefore, whatever psychical activity accompanies the visual proc- esses must be mediated through this portion of the brain. In the higher animals, however, the development of a cerebral cortex is followed by the evolution of the optic radiation, and as the con- nections of the occipital cortex increase in importance those of the midbrain (with the optic fibers) dwindle correspondingly. Here, as in other cases, the psychical activity is concentrated in the portions of the brain lying most anteriorly, and doubtless the degree of consciousness is greatly intensified in the higher animals in correspondence with the development of the cerebral cortex whose striking characteristic is its capacity to evoke a psychical reaction. The Auditory Center. — The location of the auditory area has been investigated along lines similar to those used for the visual center. The experimental physiological work has yielded varying results in the hands of different observers.- Munk and Ferrier placed the cortical center for hearing in the temporal lobe, and in spite of negative results by Schafer and others this localization has been shown to be substantially correct. Entire ablation of both temporal lobes is followed by complete deafness. Ablation on one side, however, is followed only by impairment of hearing, and in the light of the results from histology and from the clinical side it seems probable that the connections of the auditory cortex with the ear follow the general schema of the optical system rather than that of the body senses. That is, it is probable that the auditory fibers from each ear end partly on the same side and partly or mainly on the opposite side of the cerebrum. The exact portion of the temporal lobe that serves as the immediate organ of auditory sensations cannot be determined with certainty, but it seems certain that it lies mainly in the superior temporal gyrus, and the transverse gyri extending from this into the fissure of Sylvius. The Histological Evidence. — On the histological side the paths of the auditory fibers have been followed with a large measure of suc- cess, although in many details the opinions of the different investi- gators vary considerably. The eighth cranial nerve springs from the bulb by two roots: the external and the internal. The former has SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 199 been shown to supply, mainly at least, the cochlear portion of the internal ear, and is, therefore, the auditory nerve proper. This division is spoken of as the cochlear branch. The internal root sup- plies mainly the vestibular branch of the internal ear, and is there- fore spoken of as the vestibular branch (see Fig. 89) . It seems cer- tain that the latter is not an auditory nerve, but is concerned with peculiar sensations that have an important influence on mus- cular activity, especially in complex movements. The central course of these two roots is quite as distinct as their peripheral Posterior nucleus. Deiters's nucleus. Dorsal nucleus. Ventral nucleus. Cochlear branch. Vestibular branch. Semicircular canals. Scarpa's ganglion. Cochlea. Spiral ganglion. Fig. 89. — The medullary nuclei of the eighth nerve. — (From Poirier and Charpy.) distribution, — a fact that bears out the supposition that they medi- ate different functions. The central course of the cochlear branch is indicated schematically in Figs. 89 and 90. The fibers con- stituting this branch arise from nerve cells in the modiolus of the cochlea, — the spiral ganglion. These cells, like those in the posterior root ganglia, are bipolar. One axon passes peripherally to end around the sense cells of the cochlea, at which point the sound waves arouse the nerve impulses. The other axon passes toward the pons, forming one of the fibers of the cochlear branch. On entering the pons these cochlear branches end in two nuclei, one lying ventral to the 200 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. restiform body and known as the ventral or accessory nucleus (V.n., Fig. 90), and one dorsally, known as the dorsal nucleus or the tuberculum acusticum (D.n.). From these nuclei the path is continued by secondary sensory neurons, and its further course toward the brain is still a matter of much. uncertainty in regard to many of the details.* The general course of the fibers, however, is known. Those axons that arise from the accessory nucleus pass mainly to the opposite side by slightly different routes (Fig. 90). Some strike directly across toward the vental side of the pons, forming a conspicuous band of transverse fibers that has long been <£* known as the corpus trapezoideum ; others pass dorsally around the restiform body and then course downward through the tegmental region to enter the cor- pus trapezoideum. The fibers of this cross band end, according to some observers, in certain nu- clei of gray matter on the opposite side of the pons, especially in the superior olivary body and the trapezoidal nucleus, and thence the path forward is continued by a third neuron. Certainly from the level of the superior olivary body the audi- tory fibers form a dis- tinct band long known to the anatomist and des- ignated as the lateral fillet or lateral lemniscus. Authors differ as to whether the fibers of this tract arise from nerve cells in the superior olivary and neighboring nuclei, or are the fibers from the accessory nucleus which pass by the superior olivary body with- out ending and then bend to run forward in a longitudinal direc- tion. This last view is represented in the schema (Fig. 90). The secondary sensory fibers that arise in the tuberculum acusticum pass dorsally and then transversely, forming a band of fibers that comes so near to the surface of the floor of the fourth ventricle as to * For literature see Van Gehuchten, "Le Nevraxe," 4, 253, 1903. Fig. 90. — Diagram to show central course of auditory fibers (modified from Van Gehuchten) : D.n., Dorsal nucleus giving rise to the fibers that form the auditory stria? (a.s.); V.n., the ventral nu- cleus, giving origin to the fibers of the corpus trape- zoideum (,c.tr.); s.o., superior olivary nucleus; l.f., lateral fillet; n.s., nucleus of the lateral fillet; t.g.i., the inferior colliculus. SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 201 form a structure visible to the eye and known as the medullary or auditory striae. The fibers of this system dip inward at the raphe* cross the mid-line, and a part of them at least eventually reach the lateral fillet of the other side either with or without ending first around the cells of the superior olivary nucleus. According to the description of some authors, the fibers from the accessory nu- cleus and tuberculum acusticum do not all cross the mid-line to reach the lateral fillet of the other side ; some of them pass into the lateral fillet of the same side ; so that the relations of the fibers of the coch- lear nerves to the lateral fillet resemble, in the matter of crossing, the relations of the optic fibers to the optic tract. After entering the lateral fillet the auditory fibers pass forward toward the midbrain and end in part in the gray matter of the inferior colliculus, of the median or internal geniculate, and, according to Van Gehuchten, in a small mass of nerve cells in the midbrain known as the superior nucleus of the fillet. From this second or third termination an- other set of fibers, the auditory radiation, continues forward through the posterior extremity of the internal capsule to end in the superior temporal gyrus (see Fig. 77, E). According to Flechsig,* who has studied the course of these fibers in the embryo by the myeliniza- tion method, the main group passes from the internal geniculates to the transverse gyri of the temporal lobe within the fissure of Sylvius. The internal geniculates, in man at least, have therefore the func- tion of a subordinate auditory center, as the external geniculates have the function of a subordinate visual center. The internal geniculates are connected with the inferior colliculus, and also, it will be remembered, with each other, by commissural fibers (Gud- den's commissure) that pass along the optic tracts and the posterior margin of the chiasma. The auditory path, therefore, involves the following structures: The spiral ganglion, the cochlear nerve, accessory nucleus and tuberculum acusticum, corpus trapezoideum, medullary striae, superior olivary, lateral fillet, inferior colliculus, median geniculate, Gudden's commissure, auditory radiation, and temporal cortex. The Physiological Significance of the Lower Auditory Cen- ters.— The auditory path connects directly with two or three sets of nerve cells before terminating finally in the cortex. The lower centers in pons, midbrain, or thalamencephalon connect probably with motor paths through which co-ordinated reflex movements may be effected. But whether or not any perceptible degree of con- sciousness can be mediated through these lower centers remains undetermined. Goltz's dog without its cerebrum could be awak- ened from sleep by loud noises, and Schafer contends that in monkeys the removal of both temporal lobes is "notf followed by * Flechsig, "Localisation der geistigen %r£Qj^%\\Jz\pftgHs96fO T/) • 202 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. complete deafness. As in the case of vision, questions of this kind will eventually find their most satisfactory answer from the study of the results of pathological lesions in man. The Motor Responses from the Auditory Cortex. — Accord- ing to Ferrier, stimulation of the cortex of the temporal lobe (infe- rior convolution) causes definite movements, such as pricking of the ears and turning of the head and eyes to the opposite side. As in the case of the visual area, therefore, we must suppose that distinct motor paths originate in the auditory region, and it is natural to suppose that these paths give a means for cortical reflex movements following upon auditory stimulation. The Olfactory Center. — The olfactory sense is quite un- equally developed in different mammals. Broca divided them from this standpoint into two classes: the osmatic and the anosmatic group, the latter including the cetacea (whales, porpoise, dolphin) . The osmatic group in turn has been divided into the microsmatic and macrosmatic animals, the latter class including those animals in which the sense of smell is highly developed, such as the dog and rabbit, while the former includes those animals, such as man, in which this sense is relatively rudimentary.* The peripheral end- organ of smell consists of the olfactory epithelium in the upper portion of the nasal chambers. The physiology of this organ will be considered in the section on special senses. The epithelial cells of which it consists are comparable to bipolar ganglion cells. The processes or hairs that project into the nasal chamber are acted upon by the olfactory stimuli, and the impulses thus aroused are conveyed by the basal processes of the cells, the olfac- tory fibers, through the cribriform plate of the ethmoid bone into the olfactory bulb. The Olfactory Bulb and its Connections. — The olfactory bulbs are outgrowths from and portions of the cerebral hemi- spheres. Each bulb is connected with the cerebral hemispheres by its olfactory tract. The connections established by the fibers of this tract are widespread, complicated, and in part incom- pletely known. All those portions of the brain connected with the sense of smell are sometimes grouped together as the rhinen- cephalon. According to von Kolliker, the parts included under this designation are, in addition to the olfactory bulb and tract, Ammon's horn, the fascia dentata, the hippocampal lobe, the fornix, the septum lucidum, and the anterior commissure. The schematic connections of the olfactory fibers are as follows (Fig. 91) : After entering the olfactory lobe the fibers terminate in certain globular bodies, the glomeruli olfactorii (B), whose diameter varies from 0.1 to 0.3 mm. Here connections are made by contact with the dendrites * See Barker, "The Nervous System," 1899, for references to literature. SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 203 of nerve cells of the olfactory lobe, the mitral and brush cells (C). The axons of these cells pass toward the brain in the olfactory tract. Three bundles of these fibers are distinguished: (1) The precommis- sural bundle, the fibers of which terminate in part in nerve cells sit- uated in the tract itself, but, for the most part, enter the anterior commissure and pass to the same or the opposite side, to end in the hippocampal lobes or other gray matter belonging to the rhinen- cephalon. (2) The mesial bundle, the fibers of which terminate in the gray matter adjacent to the base of the olfactory tract, Fig. 91. — Diagram of the central course of the olfactory fibers: /, Olfactory bulb; //, olfactory tract; ///, cortex of the hippocampal lobe (gyrus uncinatus) ; IV, anterior commissure, olfactory portion; A, olfactory epithelial cells of nose (their fibers, olfactory nerve fibers, terminate in the glomeruli of the bulb) ; B, glomeruli of olfactory bulb where the olfactory fibers come in contact with the dendrites of the mitral cells; C, mitral and brush cells; 1, 2, 3, axons from the mitral cells constituting the fibers of the olfactory tract^ Fibers 3, which enter the commissure, arise, according to some observers, from cells in the olfactory lobe near the base of the tract. the tuberculum olfactorium, whence the path is probably continued by other neurons to the region of the hippocampal lobe. (3) The lateral tract, whose fibers seem to pass to the hippocampal lobe of the same side. According to Van Gehuchten,* none of the fibers of the anterior commissure arise from the nerve cells in the olfactory bulb. He considers that the fibers in the olfactory portion of this commissure constitute an association system connecting the olfac- tory lobe of one side with the olfactory bulb of the other side. * Van Gehuchten, "Le Nevraxe," 6, 191, 1904. 204 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. The Cortical Center for Smell. — So far as the histological evidence goes, it tends to show that the chief cortical termination of the olfactory paths is found in the hippocampal lobe, especially its distal portion, the gyrus uncinatus. The experimental evi- dence from the side of physiology points in the same direction. Ferrier states that electrical stimulation in this region is followed by a torsion of the lips and nostrils of the same side, muscular move- ments that accompany usually strong olfactory sensations. On the other hand, ablations of these regions are followed by defects in the sense of smell. The experimental evidence is not very satisfactory, owing to the technical difficulties in operating upon these portions of the brain without at the same time involving neighboring regions. There is some clinical evidence also that lesions in this region involve the sense of smell. Thus Carbonieri records that a tumor in this portion of the temporal lobe occa- sioned epileptic attacks which were accompanied by nauseating odors. The Cortical Center for Taste Sensations. — Practically nothing definite is known concerning the central paths and cortical termination of the taste fibers. The course of these fibers in the peripheral nerves has been much investigated and the facts are mentioned in the section upon "special senses." It is usually assumed, although without much decisive proof, that the cortical center lies also in the hippocampal convolution posterior to the area of olfaction. Experimental lesions in this region, according to Ferrier, are accompanied by disturbances of the sense of taste. On embryological grounds Flechsig supposes that the cortical center may lie in the posterior portion of the gyrus fornicatus (6, Fig. 94). Aphasia. — The term aphasia means literally the loss of the power of speech. It was used originally to indicate the condition of those who from accident or disease affecting the brain had lost in part or entirely the power of expressing themselves in spoken words, but the term as a general expression is now extended to include those who are unable to understand spoken or written language, — that is, those who are word-blind or word-deaf. It is usual, there- fore, to distinguish sensory aphasia from motor aphasia. By the latter term is meant the condition of those who are unable to speak, and by sensory aphasia those who are unable to understand the written, printed, or spoken symbols of words. Motor Aphasia. — A condition of motor aphasia not infrequently results from injuries to the head or from hemorrhage in the region of the middle cerebral artery. The first exact knowledge of the portion of the brain involved seems to have been obtained by Bouillaud (1825) as the result of numerous autopsies. SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 205 (It is a curious fact that Bouillaud's observations were inspired by the work of Gall. Gall having observed, as he thought, that individuals who are fluent speakers or who have retentive memories are characterized by projecting eyes, concluded that this peculiarity is due to the larger size of the lower part of the frontal lobe, and he therefore located the faculty of speech in this region of the brain. In spite of the vagaries into which he was led by his false methods Gall made many most important contributions to our knowledge of the anatomy of the brain and the cord. The discovery of the location of the center of speech, however, cannot be rightly placed to his credit, since his reasons for its location were, so far as we know, entirely unjustified. It cannot be reckoned as more than a coincidence that in this particular his phrenological localization was afterward in a measure justified by facts.) The essential truth of Bouillaud's observations was established by other observers, and Broca especially located the part of the brain involved in these lesions in the posterior part of the third or inferior frontal convolution. This region is, therefore, frequently known as Broca's convolution or Broca's center. Subsequent ob- servations have abundantly confirmed this localization, and what is designated as the "speech center" is placed in the inferior frontal convolution in the gyrus surrounding the anterior or ascending limb of the fissure Of Sylvius (£, Fig. 92.) Moreover, autopsies have shown that in right-handed persons this center is placed or is func- tional usually in the left cerebral hemisphere, while in the case of left- handed individuals aphasia and paralysis are produced by lesions involving the right side of the brain. This region is not the direct cortical motor center for the muscles of speech. It is possible that aphasia may exist without paralysis of these latter muscles. It is rather the memory center of the motor innervations necessary to form the appropriate sounds or words with which we have learned to express certain concepts. The child is taught to express certain ideas by definite words, and the memory apparatus through which these associations are transmitted to the motor apparatus may be conceived as located in the speech center. Lesions of any kind affecting this area will therefore destroy more or less the ability to use appropriately spoken words, and clinical experience shows that motor aphasia may be exhibited in all degrees of completeness and in many curious varieties. The individual may retain the power to use a limited number of words, with which he expresses his whole range of ideas, as, for instance, in the case described by Broca,* in which the individual retained for the expression of numbers only the word three, and was obliged to make this word do duty for all numerical concepts. Other cases are recorded in which the patient had lost only the power to use names — that is, nouns (Marie) — or could remember only the initial letters. Others still in which words could be used only when associated with musical memories, as in singing. It does not seem to be certain whether or not, in * Exner, "Hermann's Handbuch der Physiologie," vol. iii, part n, p. 342. Consult for older literature. 206 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. the case of complete lesion of the center on one side, that of the other can be taught to assume its function. Some recorded cases seem to indicate that this substitution is possible in the young, while in the old it is more difficult or impossible. We express our thoughts not only in spoken but also in written symbols. As this latter form of expression involves a different set of muscles, and a different educational experience, it is natural to assume that the complex associations concerned, or, to use a convenient expression, the memory centers, should involve a different part of the cortex. It is, in fact, observed that some aphasics exhibit the symptom of loss of power to write, — a condition designated as agraphia. The area in which the motor associations for the act of writing are located is placed in the middle or sec- ond frontal convolu- tion contiguous to the cortical motor centers for the muscles of the arm and hand (W, Fig. 92). Sensory Aphasia. — In sensory aphasia * (amnesia) the individ- ual suffers from an inability to under- stand spoken or writ- ten language, and as the cortical centers for hearing and seeing are situated in distinct parts of the brain, we should expect that the mechanism for the association, in one case of visual memories of verbal symbols with certain concepts and in the other case of auditory memories, should also be located in separate regions. Inability to understand spoken language, or word-deafness, is, in fact, usually attributed to a lesion involving the first temporal convolution contiguous to the cortical sense of hearing (H, Fig. 92), while loss of power to understand written or printed language, word- blindness, is traced to lesions involving the inferior parietal con- volution, the gyrus angularis, contiguous to the occipital visual center (V, Fig. 92). These two conditions may occur together, but cases are recorded in which they existed independently. It * Consult Starr, "Aphasia," "Transactions of the Congress of American Physicians and Surgeons," vol. i, p. 329, 1888. H Fig. 92. — Lateral view of a human hemisphere; cortical area V, damage to which produces "mind- blindness" (word-blindness) ; cortical area H, damage to which produces "mind-deafness" (word-deafness); cortical area S, damage to which causes the loss of audible speech ; cortical area W, damage to which abol- ishes the power of writing. — (Donaldson.) SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 207 may be imagined that the individual suffering from word-blindness alone is essentially in the condition of one who attempts to read a foreign language. The power of vision exists, but the verbal symbols have no associations, therefore no meaning. So one who is word-deaf alone may be compared to the normal individual who is spoken to in a foreign tongue. The words are heard, but they have no associations with past experience. The general facts regarding aphasia illustrate excellently the modern conception of cerebral localization. The understanding and the use of spoken or written language is, so to speak, a mental whole, both from the standpoint of education and of use. To understand or to express certain conceptions implies the use of definite words, and our visual, auditory, and motor experiences are combined in these symbols. Each phase of this complex may be cultivated more or less separately; in the case of the unlettered man, for instance, the written or printed symbols form no part in the associations connected with his verbal concepts. Corre- sponding to these facts we have, on the anatomical side, a portion of the brain in which the auditory memories are organized, — that is, connected in some way with a definite arrangement of nerve cells and their processes, another part in which the visual memories are organized, and other parts in which the motor memories as regards speaking or writing are laid down in some definite form. Each part is a distinct center, but their combined use in intellectual life would imply that they are connected by association fibers, so that, although fundamentally distinct, they are practically com- bined in their activity. Corresponding with this conception it is found from clinical experience that aphasics, although the lesion may affect only one of these various centers, suffer a deterioration, more or less pronounced, of their general intellectual capacity. We may conceive that the varying gifts of individuals, in the matter of the use of language, rest partly on the amount of train- ing received and partly on the inborn character and completeness of the nervous machinery in the different centers. The Association Areas. — According to the views presented above, it will be seen that the motor and sense areas occupy only a small portion of the cortex, forming islands, as has been said, surrounded by much larger areas. Flechsig* has designated these latter areas as association areas, and has advocated the view that they are the portions of the cortex in which the higher and more complex mental activities are mediated, the true organs of thought. His views as to the relations and physiological significance of these areas have been based chiefly on the study of the embryo brain * Flechsig, "Gehirnund Seele," Leipzig, 1896; also, "Archives de neurol- ogie," vol. ii, 1900. 208 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. with reference to the time of acquisition of the myelin sheaths. Thus he finds that the fibers to the sense areas acquire their myelin, and therefore according to his view become fully functional before those distributed to the association areas. Moreover, in the em- bryo, at least, these latter areas are not supplied with projection fibers, — that is, they are not connected directly with the under- lying parts of the nervous systems. Their connections are with the various sense centers and motor centers of the cortex. The association areas may be regarded therefore as the regions in which the different sense impressions are synthesized into complex perceptions or concepts. The foundations of all knowledge are to be found in the sensations aroused through the various sense organs; through these avenues alone can our consciousness come into relation with the external or the internal (somatic) world, and the union of these sense impressions into organized knowledge is, according to Flechsig, the general function of the association areas. This function of the association areas is indicated by the anatomical fact that they are connected with the various sense centers by tracts of association fibers, suggesting thus a mechanism by which the sense qualities from these separate sense centers may be combined in consciousness to form a mental image of a complex nature. The sequence of phenomena in the external world is or- derly, and, corresponding to this fact, the reflection of these phenom- ena in the sequence and combinations of sensations is also orderly. In the association areas our memory records of past experiences and their connections are laid down in some, as yet unknown, material change in the network of nerve cells and fibers. Here, as elsewhere in the nervous system, it may be supposed that the efficiency of the nervous machinery is conditioned partly by the completeness and character of training, but largely also by the inborn character of the machinery itself. The very marked differ- ences among intelligent and cultivated persons — for instance, in the matter of musical memory and the power of appreciating and reproducing musical harmonies — cannot be attributed to differences in training alone. The gifted person in this respect is one who is born with a certain portion of his brain more highly organized than that of most of his fellow-men. This general conception that the special capacities of talented individuals rest chiefly upon inborn differences in structure or organization of the brain may be re- garded as one outcome of the modern doctrine of localization of functions in this organ. In the beginning of the nineteenth century it seems to have been the general view that those who had a high degree of mental capacity might direct their activity with equal success in any direction according to the training received. A man who could walk fifty miles to the north, it was said, could just as SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 209 easily walk fifty miles to the south, and a man whose training made him an eminent mathematician might with different training have made an equally eminent soldier or statesman. In our day, however, with our ideas of the organization of the brain cortex, and our knowledge that different parts of this cortex give different reactions in consciousness, it seems to follow that special talents are due to differences in organization of special parts of the cortex. Subdivision of the Association Areas. — On anatomical grounds Flechsig distinguishes three (or four) association areas: The frontal or anterior (35, Fig. 95), which lies in front of the motor area; the median or insular, — that is, the cortex of the island of Reil; and the posterior, which lies back of the body fueling area, extending to the occipital lobe and also laterally into the temporal lobe. This area Flechsig suggests may be subdivided into a parietal area, 34, Fig. 95, and a temporal area, 36, Fig. 95. The greater relative development of these areas is one of the features distinguishing the human brain from those of the lower mammals. In accordance with the general conception of localization of functions Flechsig suggests that these areas have different functions, — that is, take different parts in the complex of mental activity. Basing his views upon the nature of the association tracts connecting them with the sense centers, he suggests that the posterior area is concerned particularly in the organization of the experiences founded upon visual and auditory sensations, and shows especial development in cases of talents, such as those of the musician, which rest upon these experiences. The anterior area, being in closer connection with the body sense area, may possibly be espe- cially concerned in the organization of experiences based upon the internal sensations (bodily appetites and desires). In this part of the brain possibly arises the conception of individuality, the idea of the self as distinguished from the external world. And in alterations or defective development of this portion of the brain may lie possibly the physical explanation of mental and moral degeneracy. This general idea is borne out in a measure by his- tological studies of the brains of those who are mentally deficient (amentia) or mentally deranged (dementia). It is stated* that the brain in such cases shows a distinct wasting of the cortex and that the maximum focus of this change is found in the prefrontal lobes (anterior association area). In the case of the idiotic this area is distinctly undeveloped and in the insane the atrophy is marked in proportion to the degree of dementia. Regarding the peculiar functions of the cortex of the island of Reil there are no facts sufficiently distinct to warrant even a provisional statement. * Bolton, "Brain," 1903, p. 215. 14 210 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. The area is much more developed in man than in the lower mam- mals, and its connections with other parts of the cortex by means of association tracts are such as to lead to the supposition that its general functions are of the higher synthetic character attributed to the association areas in general. The Development of the Cortical Areas. — In a recent report Flechsig* gives the results of an extensive study of the time of myelinization of the fibers in the cerebrum of man from the fourth month of intra-uterine to the fourth month of extra-uterine life. The first areas to develop in the cortex are the primary sense centers (smell, cutaneous and muscle sense, sight, hearing, and touch), and later in connection with these centers systems of motor fibers appear. There are thus formed seven primary zones, sensory and motor, to which he gives the name of projection areas. The location of these areas is shown in part in Figs. 93 and 94, 2 (26, £°), 5, 6, 7 (7b), 8, 15. Two areas connected with the olfactory sense are not shown in these figures; they appear in the anterior perforate lamina on the base of the brain and the uncinate gyrus. Later there is developed around these primary projections areas what Flechsig calls marginal or border zones, which have no projection fibers, but which are connected by short association fibers with one or more of the primary projection zones, 14, 16 to 33, in Figs. 95 and 96. These areas all develop after birth; and from a physiological standpoint may be regarded perhaps as the seat of the organized memories connected with the primary sense centers. It is injuries in these centers which may be supposed to produce the various kinds of aphasia described above. Thus, areas 17, 20, and 24 form border areas to the primary area of sight (5); 16 has the same relation to 2, 18 to 2b, and 14, ub with 7. Later still the great association areas — 34, 35, 36, Figs. 95 and 96 — acquire their myelinated fibers. These latter centers, as indicated above, may be considered as association areas with more complex connections, and they serve to mediate therefore the higher psychical activities. Flechsig, in his recent report, designates these areas from an anatom- ical point as terminal or central zones. As the result of his his- tological work, as far as it has progressed, he distinguishes thirty-six areas in the cortex in which the myelinization of the fibers occurs separately, and in which, therefore, by inference, different physiological activities are mediated. These 36 areas are subdivided as follows: * Flechsig, " Berichte der mathematisch-physischen Klasse der konigl. Sachs. Gesellschaft der Wissenschaften zu Leipzig," 1904. For a summary of the results of this work see Sabin, " The Johns Hopkins Hospital Bulletin," February, 1905. SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 211 ■0m , . +.: & Fig. 93.— Lateral surface of the brain, showing the primordial areas, both sensory and automatic, in dotted zones. — (Flechsig.) ■>**$ ftMfc # 10 Fig. 94.— Same zones on the mesial surface of the brain.— (Flechsig). 212 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. M ■M ■ ■ ■ • • ; §H j jj -^ / Jo * - .Wr , .....*■ ' , :• ' *?•> F*V: ' / • Fig. 95. — Lateral surfaces of the brain, showing the primordial and marginal zones. — (Flechsig.) T 36 Fig. 96. — Same areas on the mesial surface. — (Flechsig.) SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 213 I. Primary areas. la. Primary projection areas (1, S, 4, 5, 6, 7, 8 (15), seven or eight in number, and provided with projection fibers — sensory and motor. lb. Primary areas without projection fibers (3, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13) and apparently without association fibers. Functions un- certain. II. Association areas. IP* Intermediate or border areas, 14, 16-33, provided with short association fibers. II6, Terminal or central areas, 34, 35, 36, provided with long association fibers. The Corpus Callosum. — The corpus callosum is the most conspicuous of the bands of commissural fibers that connect one cerebral hemisphere with the other. Similar tracts of the same Fig. 97. — Diagram to show the composition of the corpus callosum as a system of com- missural fibers, without projection fibers. — (Cajal.) iu suuw me uumposinun ui uie ci missural fibers, without projection general nature are the anterior commissure, the fornix, the psalterium, etc. The position and great development of the corpus callosum has made it the object of experimental as well as anatomical investigation. When the corpus is divided by a section along the longitudinal fissure (v. Koranyi) no perceptible effect of either a motor or sensory nature is observed in the animal. When it is stimulated electrically (Mott and Schafer) from above symmetrical movements on the two sides of the body may be obtained. If the motor cortex on one side is removed stimulation in the longi- tudinal fissures causes movements only on the side controlled by the uninjured cortex. These facts are in harmony with the results of histological studies, which indicate that the fibers of the corpus callosum do not enter directly into the internal capsules to be distributed to underlying portions of the brain, but are truly 214 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. commissural and connect portions of the cortex of one hemisphere with the cortex of the other side. This relation is indicated in the accompanying diagram (Fig. 97). So far as the motor regions are concerned, there is some evidence that the connection thus es- tablished is between symmetrical parts of the cortex (Muratoff), — that is, between parts having similar functions, — and we may per- haps regard the corpus as a means by which the functional activities of the two sides of the cerebrum are associated. The Corpora Striata and Optic Thalami. — The numerous masses of gray matter found in the cerebrum beneath the cortex, in the thalamencephalon, and in the midbrain have each, of course, specific functions, but, in general, it may be said that they are intercalated on the afferent or efferent paths to or from the cortex. Their physiology is included, therefore, in the description of the functions mediated by these paths. For instance, the external geniculate bodies form part of the optic path. In addition, how- ever, these masses of cells contain in many cases reflex arcs of a more or less complicated kind, through which afferent impulses are converted into efferent impulses that affect the musculature or the glandular tissues of the body. The large nuclei constituting the corpora striata (nucleus caudatus and n. lenticularis) and the optic thalami have been frequently studied experimentally to as- certain whether they have specific functions independently of their relations to the cortex. These efforts have given uncertain results. Older experiments (Nothnagel), in which the attempt was made to destroy these nuclei by the localized injection of chromic acid, are probably unreliable, as the destruction involved also the projection fibers passing to the cortex. Lesions of the nucleus caudatus are said to be accompanied always by a rise in body temperature and an increase in heat production, and stimulation of the same nucleus gives a very marked rise in blood-pressure. These facts indicate a possible connection of this nucleus with heat and vasomotor regulation. Other observers have supposed that these nuclei are especially concerned in the co-ordination of the muscules concerned in involuntary or unconscious movements. On the anatomical side we have the striking fact that the nuclei of the corpora striata have few connections with the cortex, but, on »the other hand, send an independent system of projection fibers into the brain stem. Embryologically these structures are developed from the wall of the forebrain and would seem to have a physiological importance similar to that of the cortex itself, but experimental and clinical facts are at present insufficient to justify any hypothesis as to their special functions. With regard to the various nuclei of the optic thalamus it is known that they form abundant connections with the sensory areas of the cortex cerebri, and from this standpoint SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 215 they may be regarded as consisting of subcenters with a proba- bility, however, that reflexes may occur through them (subcortical reflexes) independently of the cortex. Numerous fibers have been traced from the thalamus to the body sense area (Flechsig). CHAPTER XI. THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBELLUM, THE PONS, AND THE MEDULLA. The functions of the cerebellum are, in some respects, less satis- factorily known than those of any other part of the central nervous system. Many theories have been held. Most of these views have been attempts to assign to the organ a single function of a definite character, but latterly the insufficiency of the theories proposed has led observers to attribute to the cerebellum general properties the nature of which can not be expressed satisfactorily in a single phrase. Before attempting to give a summary of exist- ing views it will be helpful to recall briefly the important facts re- garding its structure and relations, so far as they are known and can be used to explain its functional value. Anatomical Structure and Relations of the Cerebellum. — The finer histology of the cerebellar cortex is represented in Fig. 98. Three layers may be distinguished. The external molecular layer (A), the middle granular layer (B), and the internal medullary layer consisting of the white matter or medullated nerve fibers, afferent and efferent (C). Between the molecular and granular layers lie the large and characteristic Purkinje cells (a). The dendrites of these cells branch profusely in the molecular layer; their axons pass into the medullary layer. From the standpoint of the neuron doctrine these cells, so far as the cerebellum is con- cerned, are efferent. They form, indeed, the sole efferent system of the cerebellar cortex. The afferent fibers of the cerebellum end in both the granular and the molecular layers. Those that termi- nate in the granular layer — designated by Cajal as moss fibers, have at their terminations and points of branching curious clumps of small processes; they probably connect with the dendrites of the nerve cells in this layer. Those that pass deeper into the molec- ular layer come into connection with the dendrites of the Purkinje cells, around which, indeed, they seem to twine, so that Cajal desig- nated them as climbing fibers. The granular layer (B) contains numerous granules (g) or small nerve cells. These cells are spherical, and have a relatively large nucleus and a small amount of cyto- plasm. Their dendrites are few and short; their axons run into the molecular layer, divide in T, and the two branches then run 216 CEREBELLUM, PONS, AND MEDULLA. 217 parallel to the surface and doubtless make connections with the den- drites of the Purkinje cells as well as with the cells of the molecular layer. A few larger nerve cells of Golgi's second type (/) are found also in the granular layer. In the molecular layer are found two types of cells: the larger basket cells (b) whose axons terminate in a group of small branches that inclose the body of the Purkinje cells, and a number of smaller cells (e), situated more superficially, whose axons pass longitudinally in the molecular layer and termi- nate in arborizations or baskets that doubtless make connections with the dendrites of the Purkinje cells. Fig. 98. — Histology of the cerebellum. — (From Obersteiner.) A consideration of this peculiar and intricate structure enables us to comprehend that the cerebellar cortex presents a reflex arc of a very considerable degree of complexity. The incoming im- pulses through the moss and climbing fibers may pass at once to the Purkinje cells and lead to efferent discharges, or they may end in the cells of the granular or molecular layer and thus be distributed to the Purkinje cells in a more indirect way. In addition to the cortex the cerebellum contains several masses of gray matter in its interior: the large dentate nucleus in the center of each hemi- sphere and the group of nuclei lying in or near the middle of the 218 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. medullary substance of the vermiform lobe (nucleus fastigii, n. glo- bosi, and the n. embolif ormis) . The axons of many of the Purkinje cells of the cortex terminate in these subcortical nuclei, and the efferent path from the cerebellum is then continued by new neurons. Thus, the fibers of the superior peduncles of the cerebellum arise chiefly from the dentate nuclei, and only indirectly from the cor- tex. The anatomical connections, afferent and efferent, between the cerebellum and other parts of the nervous system are very com- plex and not yet entirely known. Without attempting to recall all of these connections, which will be found described in works upon anatomy or neurology, emphasis may be laid upon those which are at present helpful in discussing the physiology of the organ. 1. Connection with the Afferent Paths of the Cord. — The ascending fibers in the posterior columns are connected, through the nuclei of Goll and Burdach, with the cerebellum, the fibers passing by way of the inferior peduncles (restiform bodies). So also the fibers of the tracts of Flechsig and Gowers terminate eventually in the cere- bellum, as explained on p. 164. Whatever else may be their func- tions, the fibers of these tracts undoubtedly convey impulses of the muscle sense, and we have, therefore, the important fact that these fibers (as a system) terminate in part in the cerebellum and in part, by way of the median fillet, in the cerebrum. 2. Connections with the Vestibular Branch of the Eighth Cranial Nerve. — This branch, arising in the semicircular canals and utriculus and sacculus, ends in the pons in several nuclei (lateral or Deiters's, median and superior) and these in turn are connected by fibers, running probably in both directions, with the cortex of the cere- bellum and with its subcortical nuclei. This connection is most important physiologically, as it would seem to make the cerebellum a center for impulses arising in these parts of the internal ear. As we shall see, ablation of the cerebellum and destruction of the semi- circular canals give very similar results upon animals. 3. Connections with the Cortex of the Cerebrum. — A large system of fibers — cerebro-ponto-cerebellar system (see Fig. 77, A) — makes a connection between the cortex cerebri and cortex cerebelli. They emerge from the cerebellum by way of the middle peduncles, cross the mid-line, and end in gray matter (pontal nuclei) in the pons; thence their course is continued by new neurons, which pass for- ward in the crus cerebri and internal capsule to make connections with the cortex of the frontal lobe. The connection is, therefore, a crossed one, the right cerebrum being associated with the left cerebellar hemisphere. This system of fibers furnishes a mechan- ism by which the cerebellar activity may be associated with and influence the activity of the motor areas of the cerebrum. 4. Connections with the Midbrain and Cord. — The fibers of the CEREBELLUM, PONS, AND MEDULLA. 219 superior peduncles end in the red nucleus of the midbrain on the opposite side. Thence there may be connections forward with the thalamus and cerebrum; but, what is more significant, con- nections are thus made with the rubrospinal tract (Monakow's bundle), see p. 169, which descends into the cord and connects with the motor nerves. By this connection a relation may be es- tablished between the cerebellum and the motor nerves of the body. The path is doubly crossed, so that each cerebellar hemisphere is connected with the muscles of its own side. Still another motor path to the cord is made possible through the connections of the vestibular nucleus. These latter, as stated on p. 170, are connected through the vestibulospinal tract with the cells in the anterior horn. Some authors have described a special motor tract from cerebellum to cord, either direct or by way of the olivary bodies ; but our knowledge of this path is too uncertain as yet to form a positive basis for physiological conclusions. Theories Concerning the Functions of the Cerebellum. — Modern views concerning the functions of the cerebellum may be classified under three general heads: First, those that consider it a general co-ordinating center or organ for the muscular movements and especially for those concerned in equilibrium and locomotion. This view, first proposed essentially by Flourens (1824), has been adopted by many, perhaps by most, writers since his time. The manner in which the organ serves to co-ordinate these movements has been explained in various ways. According to the older ob- servers, it was supposed so to arrange or group the various motor impulses that they reached the lower motor centers in the cord in the necessary combination for co-ordinated contractions. Ac- cording to more recent observers, this synergetic action is exer- cised not directly on the motor side of the reflex but on the sensory side. The numerous sensory paths connected with the organ, especially those of the muscular sense, and those from the vestibular nerve, suggest the view that in the complex cortex of the cerebel- lum these afferent impulses act upon nervous combinations whose discharges in turn are conveyed to the motor centers in a definite and orderly sequence. Either point of view assumes that there are in the cerebellum certain distinct mechanisms — that is, combi- nations of neurons that are essentially reflex centers, and that in all of our more complex bodily movements these mechanisms intervene. The second general set of theories regarding the cere- bellum assumes that this organ is essentially the center or a center for the muscle sense. This view is connected usually with the name of Lussana,* but has been supported since in one sense or another * Lussana. See "Journal de la physiol. de rhomme," 5, 418, 1862. 220 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. by many observers.* It is, in fact, not essentially different per- haps from the second phase of the first group of theories. Those who have expressed their idea of the physiology of the cerebellum by saying that it is a center of the muscle sense have, in recent times at least, recognized that this sense has a cortical center also in the cerebrum. The view can not assume, therefore, a conscious muscle sense mediated by the cerebellum, but only that the muscle sense fibers have a cortical termination therein and that the cerebel- lar activity thus aroused is in some way necessary to the orderly adjustment of complex voluntary movements. According to an- other point of view, the cerebellum is a great augmenting organ for the neuromuscular system. It is added on, as it were, to the cere- brospinal motor system and serves not to co-ordinate the motor discharges, but to increase their strength or effectiveness. This general view, first proposed by Weir Mitchell (1869), has been sup- ported by Luys, and especially, although with important modifica- tions, by Luciani. f Some of the details of the work of the latter observer are given below. Experimental Work Upon the Cerebellum. — Rolando, and par- ticularly Flourens, gave the direction to modern experimentation in this subject. The latter observer made numerous observations, especially on pigeons, in regard to the effect of removing all or a part of the cerebellum. He describes in detail the striking results of such an operation. When all or a large part of the organ is re- moved the animal shows a most distressing inability to stand or move. There seems to be no muscular paralysis at all, but, at first, a total lack of power to co-ordinate properly the contractions of the various muscles involved in maintaining equilibrium. The animal takes a most abnormal position, with the head retracted and twisted, and any attempt to move is followed by violent disorderly contractions that may result in a series of involuntary somersaults. The animal is totally unable to fly. When the injury to the cere- bellum is less the effect upon the movements is either too slight to be noticed or is shown in a greater or less uncertainty in its movements. When it attempts to walk, for instance, it exhibits a staggering, drunken gait, a condition designated as cerebellar ataxia. Similar operations on mammals give in general the same results. If the operation is unilateral, — that is, affects only one hemisphere, — the animal (dog) exhibits forced movements, such as a tendency to roll around the long axis of his body toward the injured side and subsequently movements in a circle toward the same * See Lewandowsky, " Archiv f. Physiologie," 1903, 129. t For the literature of the cerebellum see Luciani, "II cervelleto," Florence, 1891; German translation, "Das Kleinhirn," 1893. Also Luciani, article, "Das Kleinhirn " in "Ergebnisse der Physiologie," vol. iii, part n, p. 259, 1904. CEREBELLUM, PONS, AND MEDULLA. 221 side. In man there are several cases on record in which the organ was shown by autopsy to be largely or completely atrophied and numerous cases of tumors affecting the cerebellum. In the latter group of cases there may be certain marked subjective symptoms, such as headache, and especially vertigo, and also a certain degree of ataxia or awkwardness and uncertainty of movement. So also in the cases of atrophy, in which probably the condition developed slowly through a number of years, a degree of ataxia was exhibited, especially when the movements were rapid and forced. In the ataxic condition resulting from tabetic lesions of the posterior col- umns the effect upon the movements is increased by covering up the eyes (Romberg's symptom), the individual being then deprived of his visual stimuli as well as those coming by way of the muscular and cutaneous nerves. In cerebellar ataxia, however, the effect is not increased by closure of the eyes, a result which is probably explained by the fact that the individual still possesses his paths of muscular and cutaneous sensibility to the cerebrum, and these senses may be used in the reflex adjustments of voluntary move- ments. Interpretation of the Experimental and Clinical Results. — Flourens was led by the striking results of his operations on pigeons to suggest the view that the cerebellum is an organ for the co- ordination of the movements of equilibrium and locomotion. Objections were raised to this view. Some observers (Dalton, Weir Mitchell) found that if the pigeons from which the cerebellum had been removed were kept long enough the effects first observed gradually disappeared, so that finally the animals were able to move or fly with no marked difference from the normal animal except that fatigue was shown much more quickly. Hence the view advocated by Mitchell that the essential function of the cerebellum is that of an augmenting apparatus for the voluntary movements. With regard to this view it may be remarked in passing that pigeons with the cerebral hemispheres removed exhibit apparently as a permanent symptom the same tendency to rapid fatigue after sustained muscular effort. By the same logical process therefore one might conclude that one function of the cerebrum is that of an augmenting organ to the motor discharges from the cerebellum or midbrain. So also the cases of complete or nearly complete atrophy of the cerebellum in human beings in which no evil result followed other than a slight degree of cerebellar ataxia have been used as an argument against the view that this organ is necessary to the co-ordination of the complex voluntary move- ments. The view that the cerebellum has essentially a direct co-ordinating function has been criticized most seriously by Luciani. This observer made a series of long-continued and most careful 222 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. observations upon dogs and monkeys in which the entire cere- bellum or certain definite parts had been removed. He lays stress upon the fact that the violent disturbance of movement is tem- porary and is slowly recovered from in time. He was led, therefore, to view these disturbances as due primarily not to the loss of the nor- mal functional activity of the organ, but to irritations resulting from the operation. When this stage of irritation is passed the real defects which indicate the true function of the cerebellum become apparent. These defects exhibit themselves as a loss of power in the neuromuscular apparatus of the complex voluntary move- ments, and he analyzes these results under three heads: First, a. loss of force in the muscular contractions, — a condition of asthenia; second, a loss of tone in the muscles of the limbs and trunk, par- ticularly in the hind limbs, — a condition of atonia; and, third, a loss of steadiness in the muscular contractions, — a condition of astasia. The astasia manifests itself in a tremor of the muscles when voluntarily contracted, especially in movements requiring much exertion. Luciani supposes that this tremor is due to an alteration — that is, a slowing — of the rhythm of discharges of the impulses from the motor centers. The functions of the cerebellum on his theory are expressed, therefore, by saying that it is an aug- menting organ for the activity of the neuromuscular apparatus ; and that, so far as this augmenting or strengthening activity can be ana- lyzed, it consists in an increase in the energy of the motor discharges (sthenic action), an increase in the tension or tone of the motor centers and their connected muscles (tonic action), and an increase in the rhythm of the motor impulses (static action) so that nor- mally the muscular contractions are of the nature of complete tetani. Luciani believes that this action of the cerebellum is continuous, although varying in intensity, and that it affects all of the musculature of the body, and not simply the muscles con- cerned in body equilibrium. This constant motor activity is in turn dependent upon a constant inflow of sensory impulses into the cerebellum along its afferent connections, particularly upon the impulses from the vestibular portion of the internal ear, and those from the muscle sense fibers and perhaps also from the fibers of the cutaneous senses. The constant augmenting activity of the cerebellum is therefore a species of reflex effect, — a reflex tonus which affects all the musculature. Whether the cerebellar mechan- ism is especially arranged to co-ordinate its effect upon the neuro- muscular apparatus, — that is, in some way to adapt the move- ments to a definite end — Luciani leaves an open question. He does not believe that a lack of co-ordination (cerebellar ataxia) is necessarily present in cerebellar lesions ; but admits that, if this symptom is an invariable one, it would be necessary to add to CEREBELLUM, PONS, AND MEDULLA. 223 the general augmenting activity of the cerebellum also a general adaptive or co-ordinating activity. It is precisely this latter feature which stands out in the minds of most physiologists as the characteristic function of the cerebellum, while Luciani considers that it is not demonstrated by clinical or experimental facts, and that even if demonstrated it would have to be considered as a part — perhaps a subordinate part — of the functional influence of this organ. Conclusions as to the General Functions of the Cerebel- lum.— It is evident that an authoritative statement of the function or functions of the cerebellum is impossible. It seems quite clear, however, that the organ exerts a regulating influence of some kind upon the neuromuscular apparatus of our so-called voluntary movements. The precise nature of the regulating influence is in dispute, and one who reads the literature finds it difficult at times to separate clearly the different theories proposed, since some authors are content with general statements and others attempt a more specific analysis. On the whole, it seems desirable at present to hold to the general idea, introduced by Flourens, that the cerebellum is a central organ for co-ordination of voluntary movements, particularly the more complex movements necessary in equilibrium and locomotion. Instead, however, of assuming with Flourens that the cerebellum contains a co-ordinating principle, an expression that means nothing at present, we may assume that it exerts its co-ordinating influence by virtue of the definite nervous mechanisms contained in it — that is, by nervous complexes which, on the afferent side, are connected with the peripheral sensory nerves to the vestibule of the ear, the muscles, and the skin, and on the efferent side are in direct or indirect relations with the motor areas of the brain as well as the motor centers in the cord. These mechanisms are inherited structures, but, like other nervous mechan- isms, they are developed by use. The many muscular contractions made in our ordinary movements of equilibrium are learned by ex- perience, and the effects of this training are felt mainly upon these cerebellar paths or mechanisms. We regard the speech center in the cerebrum as a collection of nervous mechanisms in which are stored or preserved the connections necessary to the motor presentation of thoughts, a memory center for the spoken symbols of our con- cepts; it is possible that in the same way we may regard the cere- bellum as a memory center of the muscular movements concerned in equilibrium. The relations of the cerebellum with the motor areas of the cerebrum and the motor centers in the cord are evidently quite complex and far from being fully understood. Moreover, this relationship must vary considerably in different animals. Removal of the cerebrum from a pigeon leaves an animal with almost perfect 224 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. power of controlling its equilibrium. In the dog a similar operation is followed by a longer period of inability to control perfectly the movements of locomotion, and it is probable that in man after such an operation the power of locomotion would be acquired more slowly, if at all. On the other hand, the violent effect upon such move- ments caused by the removal of the cerebellum in the pigeon is less evident in the dog, and, if we may judge from the incomplete data of clinical neurology, very much less evident in man. In man the motor control of the voluntary muscular system through the cere- brum is more highly developed than in the lower animals. The most important objection to the view that the cerebellum is an organ of co-ordination for the movements of equilibrium and locomotion is that in the bird as well as in man the animal eventually learns to carry out these movements after loss of the cerebellum. This fact is clear proof that the cerebellum is not the only mechanism through which such co-ordination is possible ; but it is no valid objection to the view that normally this control is effected through this organ. The sensory tracts on which this co-ordination depends make connection with the thalamus or cere- brum as well as the cerebellum, and when the latter arc is broken the higher centers may be used to replace its functions in part at least. The replacement is not complete, since even in man loss of the cerebellum is followed by a permanent condition of slight ataxia. Lewandowsky's* suggestion that normally in man the finer, more conscious movements of the body are controlled directly from the cerebrum, while the subconscious or dimly conscious movements of locomotion and equilibrium are regulated through the cerebellar centers seems to be in accord with the facts known. The Psychical Functions of the Cerebellum. — In the cerebel- lum, as in the other nerve centers below the cerebrum, we have to consider the possibility of a psychical or conscious side to the activity of the organ. It seems clear, however, that the degree of conscious- ness, if any, exhibited by the cerebellum is of a much lower order than that shown by the cerebrum. All observers agree that there is no marked loss of sensations, but Luciani, Russell, and others state their belief that in some indefinable way the mentality of the animal is affected by removal of the cerebellum. Whatever functions of this kind are present we can define only by the un- satisfactory term of subconscious rather than unconscious. As far as can be determined, this effect is felt mainly upon the muscular sense and the sense of direction. Localization of Function in the Cerebellum. — All observers agree that so far as the influence of the cerebellum on the muscula- * Lewandowsky, " Archiv f. Physiologie," 1903, 129; see also Kohnstamm, "Archiv f. d. gesammte Physiologie," 89, 240, 1902. CEREBELLUM, PONS, AND MEDULLA. 225 ture of the body is concerned, it is homolateral, — that is, each half of the cerebellum is connected with its own half of the body. The connection with the motor areas of the brain is the re- verse, the right half of the cerebrum being in relation with the left half of the cerebellum. These relations are in the main borne out by the anatomical course of the motor and sensory paths described above. But those who have operated upon the cere- bellum by the method of ablation agree entirely in the statement that by this method at least no evidence is obtained of a localization of function in the cerebellar hemispheres. There is no reason to believe that extirpation of definite areas in the hemispheres affect definite groups of muscles either on the sensory or the motor side. We are forced to conclude, therefore, that localization is absent and that regarding the cerebellum we must believe, as was formerly believed regarding the cerebrum, that each half is everywhere func- tionally equivalent. The effect of ablations is dependent not upon the part removed, but rather upon the quantity. It is to be doubted, perhaps, whether this view will stand the test of more complete investigations, for some data exist that suggest the possibility of a localization. It is observed, for instance, that the effects of ablation upon the movements of the animal are more marked the closer the injury is to the mid-line,* — that is, the more the vermi- form lobe is involved. The possibility of a more or less definite localization is suggested also by the effects of stimulation of the cerebellar cortex. Ferrier especially has described definite move- ments of the eyes, head, or limbs following electrical excitation of definite regions of the cortex; but this indication has not been developed by later experimenters with sufficient success to lead to positive conclusions. The Medulla Oblongata. — In the medulla oblongata we must recognize a region of special physiological importance in that it is the seat of certain centers which control the activity of the circulatory and respiratory organs. If the medulla is severed from the portion of the brain lying anterior to it the animal con- tinues to live for a considerable period. The respiratory move- ments are performed rhythmically, and the blood-vessels retain their tone so as to maintain an approximately normal blood-pressure. On the contrary, destruction of the medulla, or severance of its connections with the underlying parts, is followed by a cessation of respiration and a loss of tone in the arteries, either of which results in the rapid death of the organism as a whole. The portions of the medulla which exercise these important functions are desig- nated, respectively, as the respiratory and the vasomotor or vaso- constrictor centers. Their location and to some extent their con- * Lewandowsky, loc. cit. 15 226 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. /.,,. nections havabepn determined by physiological experiments, but ^^OqI ^^j^^^^^Deen possible to mark out histologically the exact ^ giuupuof*Ge1ls concerned. The position and physiological properties of these centers are described in the sections on respiration and circulation. These centers are .of especial importance because of their wide connections with the body, their essentially independent activity in reference to the higher parts of the brain, and the abso- lutely necessary character of the regulations they effect. In the development of the brain the functions originally mediated by the lower parts have been transferred more and more to the higher parts, especially in regard to conscious sensation and motion, and the so-called higher psychical activities. But the unconscious and involuntary regulation of the organs of circulation and respiration and to a certain extent of the other visceral organs has been cen- tralized, as it were, in the medulla. In addition to the control of the respiration and circulation other important reflex activities are effected through the medulla by means of the vagus nerve, which has its nucleus of origin in this part of the brain. Such, for instance, are the reflex control of the heart through the cardio- inhibitory center and of the motions and secretions of the alimentary canal. The Nuclei of Origin and the Functions of the Cranial Nerves. — The origin, course, anatomical and physiological relations of the first or olfactory, second or optic, and eighth or auditory nerves have been referred to in the preceding pages. For the sake of completeness the origin and functions of the other cranial nerves may be summarized briefly in this connection. The Third Cranial Nerve (N. Oculomotorius) . — This nerve arises from the base of the brain on the median side of the corresponding cms cerebri. It is a motor nerve supplying fibers to four of the extrinsic muscles of the eyeballs, — namely, the internal rectus, the superior rectus, the inferior rectus, and the inferior oblique — and to the levator palpebral . It innervates also two important intrinsic muscles of the eyeball, the ciliary muscle used in accommodating the eye in near vision, and the sphincter of the iris which controls in part the size of the pupil. These two latter muscles belong to the type of plain muscle, and the fibers of the third nerve which innervate them terminate in the ciliary ganglion, whence the path is continued by sympathetic nerve fibers (postganglionic fibers) to the muscles. In the interior of the brain the fibers of the third nerve arise from a conspicuous nucleus or collection of nuclei situated in the central gray matter of the midbrain at the level of the superior colliculus. The fibers for the ciliary muscle and sphincter pupillse arise more anteriorly than those for the extrinsic muscles. His- tologically three parts at least may be distinguished, as shown in *#2EL£M Fig. 99. — Nuclei of origin of motor and primary terminal sensory nuclei of cerebral nerves (Held) : Schematically represented in a supposedly transparent brain stem viewed from behind. (Nuclei and roots of motor nerves in light red, of sensory nerves in purple. Cochlear nerve in yellow.) 4, nucleus of the third nerve (n. oculomotorii); 5, nucleus of the fourth nerve (n. trochlearis); 6, the fourth nerve; 7, the descending (motor) root of the fifth nerve; 8, the principal motor nucleus of the fifth nerve; 9, the semilunar ganglion (g. Gasseri); 26, the ascending (sensory) root of the fifth nerve; 14, nucleus of the sixth cranial nerve; 15, nucleus of the facial (seventh) nerve; 16, the facial nerve; 34, 33, nucleus of the vestibular branch of the eighth cranial nerve; 32, ventral nucleus of the cochlear branch of the eighth nerve; 27, dorsal nucleus of the cochlear branch of the eighth nerve; 19, 29, the glossopharyngeal nerve; 18, 28, the vagus nerve; 20, motor nuclei of vagus and glossopharyngeal (nucleus ambiguus and nucleus dorsalis); 23, 24, nucleus of the ala? cinereaa, the solitary bundle and its nucleus; 17, the eleventh or spinal accessory nerve; 22, nucleus of the spinal accessory; 21, nucleus of the hypoglossal nerve. — (From Spalteholz, "Human Anatomy.") CEREBELLUM, PONS, AND MEDULLA. 227 Fig. 100, — namely, the lateral (or principal) nucleus, which gives origin chiefly to the fibers innervating the extrinsic muscles; the median nucleus ; and the nucleus of Edinger-Westphal. Some of the fibers, particularly those from the lateral nucleus to the infe- rior rectus, the internal rectus, and the inferior oblique, cross the mid-line and emerge in the nerve of the opposite side. The Fourth Qranial Nerve (N. Trochlearis) . — This nerve emerges from the brain in the anterior medullary velum (valve of Vieussens) just posterior to the inferior colliculus. It curves around the cms cerebri to reach the base of the brain. It is a motor nerve, and Nucleus of com. Edinger-Westphal nucleus. Principal nucleus*. Median nucleus. Nucleus of 4th nerve. Fig. 100- — Nuclei of origin of the third and fourth nerves. — (From Poirier and Charpy.) supplies fibers to the superior oblique muscle of the eyeball. In the interior of the brain the fibers arise from a nucleus in the central gray matter just posterior to that of the third nerve (Fig. 100). The fibers pass dorsalward toward the velum and make a com- plete decussation before emerging. The Fifth Cranial Nerve (N. Trigeminus). — This nerve arises from the side of the pons by two roots, a small motor root, portio minor, and a large sensory root, portio major. It is, therefore, a mixed motor and sensory nerve, supplying motor fibers to the muscles of mastication and sensory fibers of pressure, pain, and temperature to the face, the forepart of the scalp, the eye, nose, 228 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. portions of the ear, mouth, and tongue, and to the dura mater (Fig. 101). In the interior of the brain the motor portion, Fig. 101. — Diagram showing the average area of distribution of the sensory fibers of the trigeminal nerve. — (Cushing.) N. opht. Accessory motor nucleus. N. max. sup. N. max. inf. Fig. 102. — Nuclei of origin of the fifth cranial nerve. — (From Poirier and Charpy, after Van Gehuchten.) portio minor, arises partly from a small nucleus in the pons and partly from a long column of cells extending along the lower margin of the central gray matter throughout the midbrain. This column and CEREBELLUM, PONS, AND MEDULLA. 229 the fibers arising from it constitute the descending motor root of the fifth nerve (see Fig. 102). The sensory fibers originate from the nerve cells in the Gasserian ganglion (g. semilunare). The branch that enters the brain ends partly in a collection of cells in the pons, the so-called sensory nucleus, and partly in a column of cells extending posteriorly throughout the length of the medulla. These cells and the fibers ending in them constitute the descending spinal root of the fifth nerve (see Fig. 99) . The Sixth Cranial Nerve (N. Abducens). — This nerve arises from the base of the brain at the posterior edge of the pons. It is a motor nerve, and supplies fibers to the external rectus muscle of the eye- ball. In the interior of the brain its fibers originate in a small spheri- cal nucleus lying beneath the floor of the fourth ventricle. Con- nections have been traced between this nucleus and the pyramidal tract of the opposite side (Fig. 99). The Seventh Cranial Nerve (N. Facialis). — This nerve appears on the base of the brain at the posterior margin of the pons, lateral and somewhat posterior to the emergence of the sixth nerve. It is mainly a motor nerve, but carries some sensory fibers (fibers of taste and general sensibility) received through the n. intermedins of Wrisberg. The motor fibers of the nerve supply the muscles of the face, part of the scalp, and the ear, including its intrinsic muscles, and in addition secretory fibers are supplied to the submaxillary and sublingual glands. Within the brain these fibers arise from a conspicuous nucleus in the tegmental region of the pons lying ventral to the nucleus of the sixth, beneath the middle of the fourth ventricle (Fig. 99). The sensory fibers of the nerve of Wrisberg originate in the nerve cells of the geniculate ganglion. The Ninth Cranial Nerve (N. Glossopharyngeus) arises from the side of the medulla, — the restiform body. It is a mixed nerve, supplying motor fibers to the muscles of the pharynx and the base of the tongue and secretory fibers to the parotid gland. Within the brain these fibers arise from two motor nuclei common to this and the tenth nerve, — namely, a dorsal nucleus below the floor of the fourth ventricle and a smaller ventral nucleus, n. ambiguus, in the reticular substance of the tegmentum (Fig. 99). The sensory fibers supply in part the mucous membrane of the tongue and pharynx, the tympanic cavity, and the Eustachian tube. These fibers arise from cells in the two ganglia on the trunk of the nerve, the ganglion superius and g. petrosum. The branches from these cells that pass into the medulla terminate in the nucleus of the ala cinerea. The Tenth Cranial Nerve (N. Vagus or Pneumogastricus) . — This nerve arises from the side of the medulla posterior to the origin of the glossopharyngeal nerve. It is also a mixed nerve, with an 230 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. extensive distribution to the respiratory and digestive organs and the heart. Its efferent or motor fibers arise within the brain from the same masses of cells that give rise to the motor fibers of the glossopharyngeal. These fibers supply the intrinsic muscles of the larynx, esophagus, stomach, small intestine, and part of the large intestine. Inhibitory fibers are carried to the heart and secretory fibers to the gastric and pancreatic glands. Its sensory or afferent fibers are distributed to the mucous membrane of the larynx, trachea, and lungs, and to the mucous membrane of the esophagus, stomach, intestines, and gall-bladder and ducts. These fibers arise from cells in the ganglia on the trunk of the nerve, the gan- glion jugulare and g. nodosum. The branches from these cells that pass into the medulla terminate in the gray matter of the ala cinerea. The Eleventh Cranial Nerve (N. Accessorius) . — This nerve is usually described as arising by upper roots from the medulla, and by a series of lower roots from the spinal cord as low as the fifth to the seventh cervical segment. It is a motor nerve, supplying fibers to the sternomastoid and trapezius muscles. The medullary branches arise from the posterior portion of the dorsal motor nucleus which gives origin to the vagus, while the spinal branches originate from cells in the anterior horn of the gray matter of the cord (Fig. 99). The Twelfth Crainal Nerve (N. Hypoglossus) . — This nerve arises from the medulla in the furrow between the anterior pyramid and the olivary body. It is a motor nerve, supplying the muscles of the tongue and the extrinsic muscles of the larynx and hyoid bone. Within the brain these fibers originate from a distinct nucleus lying in the floor of the fourth ventricle near the mid-line (Fig. 99). CHAPTER XII. THE SYMPATHETIC OR AUTONOMIC NERVOUS SYSTEM. The chain of nerve ganglia extending on each side of the spinal column to the coccyx is known as the sympathetic nervous system. This name was given to the structure under the misapprehension that it constitutes a nerve pathway through which so-called sym- pathetic— or, as we now designate them, reflex actions of distant organs are effected. It was supposed to arise from the brain by branches connected with the fifth and sixth cranial nerves.* We now know that this system consists of a series of ganglia or col- lections of nerve cells connected with each other and connected also with the spinal nerves. Strictly speaking, the term sympathetic system is applicable only to the chain of ganglia which begins with the superior cervical ganglion at the base of the skull and ends with the ganglion coccygeum. There are, however, other outlying nerve ganglia with or without specific names which from a physio- logical and indeed from an anatomical standpoint belong to the same group. In the abdomen we have the so-called prevertebral ganglia, the semilunar ganglion from which arises the celiac plexus, the superior mesenteric, and the inferior mesenteric ganglion giving rise to the hypogastric nerve. These ganglia lie ventral to the sympathetic trunk, but are in direct connection with it. In the head region the ciliary, sphenopalatine, and otic ganglia are also of the same type. More peripherally are numerous other ganglia lying in or around the various visceral organs, such as the submaxillary ganglion near the duct from the corresponding gland, the cardiac ganglia in the heart, and the extensive system of nerve cells in the walls of the alimentary canal known as the plexuses of Meissner and Auerbach. With the exception, perhaps, of this last system, whose histological structure and connections are not satisfactorily known, all of these ganglia are frequently designated as sympathetic, and from a physiological as well as an anatomical standpoint may be considered with the ganglia of the sympathetic trunk or chain. Langley, who has contributed greatly to our knowledge of the finer anatomy and the physiology of this system, has recently proposed a different classification.! * Charles Bell, " The Nervous System of the Human Body," third edi- tion, London, 1844, p. 9. fSchafer's "Text-book of Physiology," 1900, vol. ii ; " Ergebnisse der Physiologie," 1903, vol. ii, part n, p. 823 ; also " Brain," 1903, vol. xxvi. 231 232 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. Autonomic Nervous System. — According to Langley, the efferent fibers from the nerve cells of the sympathetic and re- lated ganglia supply the plain muscle tissues, the cardiac muscles, and the glands, — that is, the organs of the involuntary or, according to an old nomenclature, the vegetative processes of the body. He proposes for this entire sys- tem of efferent fibers the term autonomic, to indicate that they possess a certain independ- ence of the central nervous system. The au- tonomic system is contrasted physiologically and anatomically with the efferent spinal and cranial fibers that supply the striated or volun- tary muscles: physiologically in the fact that this latter group of fibers is entirely dependent upon activities of the central nervous system, and anatomically in the fact that the auto- nomic fibers, although arising ultimately from the central nervous system, all pass to their pe- ripheral tissues by way of sympathetic nerve cells. The autonomic path consists of two neurons: one in the central nervous system the axon of which emerges in one of the spinal or cranial nerves and ends around the dendrites of a sympathetic cell; and one, the axon from the sympathetic cell which passes to the periph- eral tissue. The first axon is spoken of as the preganglionic fiber, the second as the post- ganglionic fiber. Their connections are repre- sented in the accompanying schema. (Fig. 103.) Physiological and anatomical investigations have shown that autonomic nerve fibers arise from four regions in the central nervous system (Fig. 104) : First, from the midbrain, emerging oeuiolL * fore. ■nwsele. t Fig. 103. — Schema to show the general relation between :_ fit the preganglionic and postganglionic paths. ibers of the autonomic Fig. 104.— Illus- trating the central ori- gin of the autonomic fibers.— (Langley.) SYMPATHETIC NERVOUS SYSTEM. 233 in the third cranial nerve and passing via the ciliary ganglion; second, from the bulbar region, emerging in the seventh, ninth, and tenth cranial nerves; third, from the thoracic spinal nerves (first thoracic to fourth or fifth lumbar) and passing in general via the ganglia of the sympathetic chain; fourth, from the sacral region by way of the so-called nervus erigens supplying the descending colon, rectum, anus, and genital organs. The autonomic fibers at their origin in the central nervous system — that is, while pregan- glionic fibers — are all possessed of a small medullated sheath, having a diameter of 1.8 ju to 4 p.. The postganglionic fiber is in most cases non-medullated, but this is by no means an invariable rule. In many cases the axons from sympathetic cells possess distinct, although small, myelin sheaths. The Nicotin Method. — The course of the autonomic fibers has been traced in many cases to their corresponding sympathetic nerve cells partly by the method of secondary degeneration and partly by the use of nicotin, as first described by Langley and Dickinson.* These authors have shown that after the use of nicotin, either injected into the circulation or painted upon the ganglion, stimulation of the preganglionic fiber in any part of its course fails to give any response, while stimulation of the post- ganglionic fiber, on the contrary, is still effective. It would seem, therefore, that the nicotin paralyzes the connection of the pre- ganglionic fiber with the sympathetic nerve cell, and by means of the local application of the drug it is possible in many cases to pick out the ganglion in which the preganglionic fiber really ends. For it often happens that in the sympathetic trunk this fiber will pass through several ganglia before making final connec- tions with the sympathetic cells. So far, the course of these fibers has been traced most successfully in the case of those sup- plying the sweat glands, blood-vessels, and especially the erector muscles of the hairs, the so-called pilomotor nerve fibers. The visible result of stimulation in the last case gives a ready means of determining the presence of the fibers. General Course of the Autonomic Fibers Arising from the Spinal Nerves. — It has long been known that the spinal nerves are connected with many of the ganglia of the sympathetic chain by fine branches known as the rami communicantes. In the tho- racic and lumbar regions (first thoracic to second or fourth lumbar) these rami consist of two parts: a white and a gray ramus, the difference in color being due to the fact that the white rami are composed almost entirely of medullated fibers, while the gray rami are largely non-medullated. In the cervical, lower lumbar, and sacral regions the rami consist only of the gray part. Physiological * " Proceedings, Royal Society," 1889, 46, 423. 234 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. experiments show that the white rami consist of preganglionic fibers that arise from nerve cells in the spinal column, pass out by way of the anterior roots, enter the white ramus, and thus reach the sympathetic chain. On entering this latter the fiber may not end at once in the ganglion at which it enters, but may pass up or down in the chain for some distance. Eventually, however, it ends around a sympathetic nerve cell and the path is then con- tinued by the axon from this cell as the postganglionic fiber. The gray rami consist of these latter fibers, which return from the sym- pathetic chain to the spinal nerves and are then distributed to the areas supplied by these nerves, particularly the cutaneous areas, since the skin branches are the ones that supply the sweat glands, the blood-vessels, and the erector muscles of the hairs. It will be noted that the fibers that pass from a given spinal nerve — say, the twelfth thoracic — by a white ramus to enter the sympathetic chain do not return as postganglionic fibers by the gray ramus to the same spinal nerve. On the contrary, the gray ramus of the twelfth thoracic may consist of the postganglionic portion of autonomic fibers that enter the sympathetic through a white ramus of one of the higher thoracic nerves. In general, we may say that there is a great outflow of autonomic fibers, including vasomotor, sweat, and pilomotor fibers, in the white rami commu- nicantes from the first or second thoracic to the second or fourth lumbar nerves. Those of these fibers that are to be distributed to the skin areas of the body — head, limbs, and trunk — return by way of the gray rami to the various spinal nerves and are distributed with these nerves, the distribution being somewhat different in different animals and for the different varieties of fibers. Those fibers that are distributed eventually to the blood-vessels, glands, and walls of the viscera have a different course from those supplying the glands, blood-vessels, and plain muscle of the head region. For the head region the fibers after entering the sympathetic chain pass upward along the cervical sympathetic to end in the superior cervical ganglion; thence the path is continued by postganglionic fibers which emerge by the various plexuses that arise from this ganglion. For the abdominal and pelvic viscera the fibers (particu- larly the rich supply of vasoconstrictor fibers), after entering the sympathetic chain, emerge, still as preganglionic fibers, by the splanchnic nerves that run to the celiac ganglion or in the branches connecting with the inferior mesenteric ganglia, and then become postganglionic fibers (see Fig. 105). The details of the course of the vasomotor, sweat, visceromotor fibers to the different regions, the cardiac fibers, etc., will be given in the appropriate sections. General Course of the Autonomic Fibers Arising from the Brain. — These fibers leave the brain in the third, seventh, ninth, SYMPATHETIC NERVOUS SYSTEM. 235 tenth, and eleventh cranial nerves nerve end, as preganglionic fi- bers, in the ciliary ganglion. Their postganglionic fibers leave this ganglion in the short ciliary nerves and innervate the plain muscle of the sphinc- ter of the iris and the ciliary muscle. The fibers that emerge in the seventh and ninth nerves probably supply the glands and blood-vessels (vaso- dilator fibers) of the mucous membrane of the nose and mouth. Some of these fibers reach the fifth nerve by way of anastomosing branches and are distributed with it. Their preganglionic portion termi- nates in some of the ganglia belonging to the sympathetic type which are found in this region, such as the sphenopal- atine and otic ganglia, and the submaxillary and sublingual ganglia for the fibers dis- tributed to the glands of the same name. The autonomic fibers that arise with the tenth (and the eleventh) nerves are distributed through the vagus. Physiologically these fibers consist of motor fibers (vis- ceromotor fibers) to the mus- culature of the esophagus, stomach, small intestine, and large intestine as far as the descending colon, motor fibers to the bronchial musculature, inhibitory fibers to the heart, and secretory fibers to the gastric and pancreatic glands. The ganglia in which the pre- ganglionic portion ends have not been definitely isolated, Those that emerge in the third Fig. 105. — Diagram giving a schematic representation of the course of the autonomic (sympathetic) fibers arising from the thoraci co- lumbar and sacral regions of the cord. The preganglionic fiber is represented in red, the postganglionic in black lines. The arrows in- dicate the normal direction of the nerve im- pulses or nerve conduction. S.c, Superior cervical ganglion ; I.e., inferior cervical gan- glion; T, the first thoracic ganglion; Sp., the splanchnic nerve; C, the semilunar or celiac ganglion ; m., the inferior mesenteric ganglion ; h., the hypogastric nerves; N.E., the nervus erigens. The numerals indicate the corre- sponding spinal nerves. 236 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. but probably they comprise the small and, for the most part, un- named local ganglia found in or near the organs innervated. General Course of the Autonomic Fibers Arising from the Sacral Cord.— The autonomic fibers of this region emerge from the cord in the anterior roots of the sacral nerves, — second to fourth. The branches from these roots unite to form the so-called nervus erigens (pelvic nerve), which loses itself in the pelvic plexus without making connections with the sympathetic chain of ganglia. The pelvic plexus is formed in part also from the hypogastric nerve arising from the inferior mesenteric ganglion. Through this latter path autonomic fibers from the upper lumbar region enter the plexus (Fig. 105). The autonomic fibers of the nervus erigens supply vasodilator fibers to the external genital organs, and in the male constitute the physiological mechanism for erection; whence the name. They supply, also, vasodilator fibers to rectum and anus and motor fibers to the plain muscles of the colon de- scendens, rectum, and anus. The preganglionic part of these fibers ends in small sympathetic ganglia in the pelvic plexus or in the neighborhood of the organs supplied. Normal Mode of Stimulation of the Autonomic Nerve Fibers. — In distinction from the nerve fibers innervating the skeletal muscles practically the whole set of autonomic fibers is removed from the control of the will. An apparent exception to this general statement is found in the fact that the ciliary muscle of the eye is seemingly under voluntary control. We must suppose that under normal conditions they are always excited reflexly, and the course of the afferent fibers concerned in these reflexes and the nature of the effective sensory stimulus in each case are important in the con- sideration of each of the physiological mechanisms involved. Most of these mechanisms, as we shall find, work reflexly — that is, without voluntary initiation — and, for the most part, uncon- sciously,— for instance, the movements of the intestines, the secre- tion of the digestive glands, and the contraction and dilatation of the arteries. The autonomic nerve fibers control, therefore, the unconscious co-ordinated actions, the so-called vegetative processes, of the body. There is no apparent reason in the anatomical ar- rangements why these fibers should be free from voluntary control. Their distinguishing characteristic in comparison with the nerves for the voluntary movements is the fact that they all terminate first in sympathetic nerve cells; but this fact gives no explanation of the absence of conscious control by the will. We are justified in saying that nerve paths that pass through sympathetic nerve cells cannot be excited voluntarily; but the immediate reason for this fact is probably to be found in the ultimate point of origin of these paths in the central nervous system. What we designate as vol- SYMPATHETIC NERVOUS SYSTEM. 237 untary motor paths arise in a definite region of the cortex, — the motor area in the frontal lobe. Our motor conceptions or ideas can affect the efferent paths arising in this region, but not those, apparently, which originate in other parts of the brain. CHAPTER XIII. THE PHYSIOLOGY OF SLEEP. The state of more or less complete unconsciousness which we designate as sleep forms a part of the physiology of the brain which naturally has attracted much attention, and the theoretical explana- tions that have been advanced at one time or another are exceed- ingly numerous. The same condition occurs in many, if not all, of the other mammalia, and, indeed, in all living things there occur periods of rest alternating with periods of activity. Whether these periods of rest are essentially similar in nature to sleep in man is a question in general physiology that can be solved only when we know more of the chemistry of living matter. Within the human body there are other tissues that exhibit periods of rest alternating with periods of activity, — the gland cells, for example. The secret- ing cells of the pancreas have a period of activity in which the destructive processes exceed the constructive, and a period of rest in which these relations are reversed. We may compare this con- dition in the gland cells with that in the brain. Sleep, from this standpoint, is a period of comparative rest or inactivity, during which the constructive or anabolic processes are in excess of the disassimilatory changes. The period of sleep is a period of re- cuperation, and doubtless all tissues have these alternating phases. To explain sleep fundamentally, therefore, it would be necessary to understand the chemical changes of anabolism and catabolism, and an explanation of the sleep of the brain tissues would doubtless explain the similar phenomenon in other tissues. But what the physiologists desire first, and have attempted to determine, is an explanation of why this condition comes on with a certain periodical regularity, — an explanation, in other words, of the mechanism of sleep, the change or changes in the brain or the body which reduce the metabolism of the brain tissue to such an extent that it falls below the level necessary to cause conscious- ness. Physiological Relations during Sleep. — The central and most important fact of sleep is the partial or complete loss of conscious- ness, and this phenomenon may be referred directly to a lessened metabolic activity in the brain tissue, presumably in the cortex cerebri. During sleep the following changes have been recorded: 238 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF SLEEP. 239 The respirations become slower and deeper and the costal respiration, (respiration by elevation of the ribs) predominates over the ab- dominal or diaphragmatic respiration as compared with the waking condition. The respiratory movements also show frequently a tendency to become periodic, — that is, to increase and decrease regularly in groups after the manner of the Cheyne-Stokes type of breathing. The expiration is frequently shorter and more audi- ble than in the respirations of the waking hours. The eyeballs roll upward and inward and the pupil is constricted. According to Lombard's observations, the knee-kick decreases or disappears entirely during sleep. Some of the constant secretions are dimin- ished in amount, — as, for instance, the urine, the tears, and the secretion of the mucous glands in the nasal or pharyngeal mem- brane. One of the familiar signs of a sleepy condition is the dryness of the surface of the eyes, a condition that leads to the rubbing of the eyes. It is sometimes stated that the digestive secretions are diminished during sleep, but the statement does not seem to rest upon satisfactory observations, and may be doubted. The pulse-rate decreases during sleep and there are also certain sig- nificant changes in the distribution of blood in the body owing to a diminished vascular tone in the skin vessels. These latter changes will be referred to more in detail below. The physiological oxidations are also decreased, as shown by the diminished output of carbon dioxid. On the whole, however, the physiological activities of the body go on much as in the waking condition. Those changes in activity that do occur are, in the main, an indirect result of the partial or complete cessation of activity in the brain. One might say that while the cortex of the brain sleeps — that is, is inactive — most of the other organs of the body may be awake and maintain their normal activity. Another fact of interest is that the entire cortex does not fall asleep at the same instant nor always to the same extent. Ordinarily as sleep sets in the power to make conscious movements is lost first and the auditory sen- sibility last, and on awakening the reverse relation holds. The individual may be conscious of sound sensations before he is sufficiently awake to make voluntary movements. The Intensity of Sleep. — The intensity of sleep — that is, the depth of unconsciousness — has been studied by the simple device of ascertaining the intensity of the sensory stimulus necessary to awaken the sleeper. Kohlschiitter * used for this purpose a pendu- lum falling against a sounding plate. At intervals of a half-hour during the period of sleep the auditory stimuli thus produced were increased in intensity until waking was caused. His results are expressed in the curve shown in Fig. 106, in which the intensity * Kohlschiitter, "Zeitschrift f. rationelle Medicin," 1863. 240 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. of the sleep is represented by the height of the ordinates. According to this curve, the greatest intensity is reached about an hour after the beginning, and from the second to the third hour onward the depth of sleep is very slight. The activities of the brain lie just below the threshold of consciousness. It appears also from this curve that the recuperative effect of sleep is not proportional to its intensity. The long period from the third to the eighth hour, in which the depth of sleep is so slight is presumably as important in restoring the brain to its normal waking irritability as the deeper period up to the third hour. It is probable that the curve of in- tensity of sleep varies somewhat with the individual and also with surrounding conditions. That individual variations occur is indi- STRENGTH OF STIMULUS 800 700 / \ / GOO 500 400 ■ / • ■ 300 ■ \ 200 100 \ \ ' HOURS 0,5 W L5 2.0 2.5 3.0 3.5 4.0 45 5.0 5.5 6.0 65 7.0 7.5 7.8 Fig. 106. — Curve illustrating the strength of an auditory stimulus (a ball falling from a height) necessary to awaken a sleeping person. The hours marked below. The tests were made at half-hour intervals. The curve indicates that the distance through which it was necessary to drop the ball increased during the first hour, and then diminished, at first very rapidly, then slowly. — (Kohlschutter.) cated by the results obtained by two other observers, Monninghoff and Piesbergen,* who used the same general method as was em- ployed by Kohlschutter. The sleeper was awakened by auditory stimuli produced by dropping a lead ball from varying heights upon a lead plate. Only two experiments were made each night, and the curves constructed represent, therefore, composites from several periods of sleep. One of the curves obtained is represented in Fig. 107. According to this curve, the maximum intensity is reached between the first and second hours, and between the fourth and the fifth hour there is a second slight increase in intensity, giving a second maximum in the curve. This latter feature of a * Monninghoff and Piesbergen, "Zeitschrift f. Biologie," 19, 1, 1883. THE PHYSIOLOGY OF SLEEP. 241 ♦IT | I /I / I 1 \ I m \ \ IMP *?T MP 175 \ \ ?in \ \ t!< \ ] \ ZOO / 1 1 | fW / 1 t!W (IS /no 75 | / V 1 **s u V \ -r._ J / tf A30 M 1 275 J w 2 V j IS 3. 10 3 V A \ ii S 43 i 4 fa J 9 J W 5 *5 < 6 41 Fig. 107. — Curve of intensity of sleep according to Mdnninghoff and Piesbergen. The figures along the abscissa represent time in hours from the beginning of sleep ; those along the ordinate the relative intensity of sleep measured in milligram-millimeters, expressing the intensity of sound of a falling body necessary to awaken the sleeper. 1300 i \ IZOO \ \ 1100 \ \ 1000 \ / 1 900 1 800 / \ / \ 700 / \ j / \ 600 / \ / / V SOO / / \ \ 400 / \ / / V 300 / \ / * y v / A 100 7 Z 3 <± S 6 7 8 9 10 // k Fig. 108. — Curve of intensity of sleep in a child of three years and eight months, as deter- mined by Czerny. 16 242 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. second increase in intensity toward morning is very apparent also in some interesting curves obtained by Czerny from children of different ages. His method of awakening the sleeper was to use induction shocks of varying intensities. In children of four years with a normal period of sleep of about twelve hours the curve shows a very marked increase in intensity toward morning, as shown in Fig. 108. Curves made by similar experimental methods are reported by Howell and by Michelson.* The striking feature about all the curves is the sharp increase in intensity shortly after falling asleep, the maximum being reached at the first or second hour of slumber. Subsequently the curve again falls rapidly and the sleep is light, but may show a greater or less increase in intensity toward the end of the period. Changes in the Circulation during Sleep. — That the circula- tion undergoes distinct and characteristic changes during sleep has been shown upon man by phlethysmographic observations and upon the lower animals by direct kymographic experiments. Using very young dogs, Tarchanofff has been able to measure their blood-pressure while sleeping. He finds that the pressuer in the aorta falls by an amount equal to twenty to fifty millimeters of mercury during sleep, and that the same general fact is true for man is shown by the sphygmomanometric observations reported by Brush and Fayerweather.J Making use of patients with a trephine hole in the skull, Mosso § has been able to show that during sleep the volume of the brain diminishes, w7hile that of the arm or foot increases. The apparent explanation of this fact is that the blood-vessels in the body dilate, and receive, therefore, more blood, while a smaller amount flows to the brain. The volume of the foot or hands was measured in these experiments by incasing it in a plethysmograph (see section on circulation). The author || has extended these observations so as to obtain a plethysmographic record of the volume of the hand and part of the forearm during a period of normal sleep. One of the records thus obtained is given in Fig. 109. The amount of dilatation is given by the ordinates below the base line. Granting that the increase in volume of the hand and arm is caused by an increase in the volume of blood contained in their blood-vessels, the curve shows that during and after the onset of sleep the blood-vessels in the arm slowly dilate until between one and two hours after the beginning of sleep. * Howell, "Journal of Experimental Medicine," 2, 313, 1897. Michel- son, "Dissertation," Dorpat, 1891. t Tarchanoff, "Archives italiennes de biologie," 21, 318, 1894. J Brush and Fayerweather, "American Journal of Physiology," 5, 199, 1901. § Mosso, "Ueber den Kreislauf des Blutes im menschlichen Gehirn," 1881. il Howell, loc. cit. THE PHYSIOLOGY OF SLEEP. 243 After this maximum is reached the arm remains more or less of the same volume for a certain period or else diminishes in volume very gradually. Shortly before waking, however, the arm begins to diminish „ more rapidly in size, owing doubt- $ less to the contraction of its blood- c vessels; so that at the time of awak- ing it has practically the same volume § as at the beginning of sleep. If, on <| the basis of Mosso's experiments, g quoted above, we assume that the § blood-flow in the brain stands in a | reciprocal relation to that in the arm, g this curve may be taken to indicate 3 that before and after the onset of sleep 2, the blood-flow through the brain di- g minishes rapidly to a certain point £ and that before awaking the blood-flow | begins to increase again until it reaches •£ normal proportions. | Effect of Sensory Stimulation. — That sensorv stimuli of various kinds =r J CD affect a sleeping individual without g- entirely awaking him is shown by the | movements that may be caused in this § way, and also by the nature of the J dreams which may be provoked. It is « very interesting to find from plethys- £ mographic observations that all kinds |- of sensory stimulations from with- g out and from within are liable to t affect the circulation of the blood • during sleep. As shown by the plethys- § mograph, the volume of the arm dimin- r ishes more or less in proportion to the ^ intensity of the stimulus, and the g probable interpretation of this fact is g- that the sensory stimulus acts reflexly S" upon the vasomotor center in the ^ medulla and causes through it a con- g traction of the blood-vessels. In the curve shown in Fig. 109 most of the irregularities were traceable to causes of this kind, — noises in the building or street or other sensory stimuli. The same fact is exhibited in a striking way by the curves given in 244 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. Fig. 110. In these experiments the recorder attached to the plethysmograph to register the changes in volume was of a differ- ent kind (tambour) and the record reads in a reverse way to that shown in Fig. 109, — that is, a dilatation is recorded by a rise in the curve and a constriction by a fall. The recorder being more sensitive, the volume changes in the arm due to the heart beat are clearly indicated. The legends attached to the illustration explain the results of the experiments. Theories of Sleep. — Many hypotheses have been advanced to explain the nature and causation of sleep. Confining ourselves to the more recent hypotheses that attempt to explain the immediate Fig. 110. — Sleep: A, effect of external impression (music box), insufficient to awaken sleeper,: — a marked diminution in volume of the arm; B, effect of external impression (music box) insufficient to awaken sleeper; a stronger diminution in volume followed by dilatation as the subject again fell asleep. cause of the production of the condition, the following brief de- scription will suffice to show the nature of the theories proposed : 1. The Accumulation of Acid Waste Products. — Preyer* and also Obersteiner have suggested that the accumulation of acid waste products in the blood brings on a gradually increasing loss of irritability or fatigue in the brain cells which results finally in a depression of their activity sufficient to cause unconsciousness. It is known that functional activity in the muscle is accompanied by the formation of acid waste products, especially sarcolactic acid, * Preyer, "Centralblatt f. d. med. Wiss.," 13, 577, 1875; and Obersteiner, "Allgemeine Zeitschrift f. Psychiatrie," 29, 224, 1S72-73. THE PHYSIOLOGY OF SLEEP. 245 and that if not removed as quickly as formed these products cause a diminution and finally a loss of irritability. The central nerve tissues in activity show also an acid reaction. Moreover, if lactic acid or its sodium salt is injected into the blood it brings on a con- dition of fatigue and finally a state of unconsciousness. The theory, therefore, supposes that during the waking hours the constant activity of the muscles and nervous system results in a gradual accumulation of these waste products, since their oxidation and removal does not keep pace with their production. The end- result is a diminishing irritability of the central nervous system, especially perhaps of the cortex, which results finally in invol- untary sleep, although normally the accumulation is not carried to this extreme, since it is our habit to induce sleep, when the sensations of sleepiness become apparent, by withdrawing ourselves from excitations, mental or sensory. 2. Consumption of the Intramolecular Oxygen. — Pfliiger* suggests that the cause of sleep lies essentially in the fact that the brain cells during the waking hours use up their store of oxygen more rapidly than it can be replaced by the absorption of oxygen from the blood. The result is a gradual reduction in irritability; so that when external stimuli are withdrawn the oxidations in the cells sink below the level necessary to arouse consciousness. During sleep the store of intramolecular oxygen — that is, the oxygen syntheti- cally combined by anabolic processes to form the irritable living matter — is again replenished. 3. The Neuron Theory. — Duval, f Cajal, and others have applied the neuron doctrine to explain the occurrence of sleep. According to the neuron conception, the connection between the cells in the cortex and the incoming impulses along the afferent paths is made by the contact of the terminal arborizations of the* afferent fibers with the dendrites of the cell. Assuming that these latter processes are contractile, Duval supposes that sleep is caused mechanically by their retraction, which results in breaking the connections and thus withdrawing the brain cells from the possibility of external stimulation. Conductivity is re-established upon awaking by the elongation and intermingling of the processes again re-establishing physiological connections. The numerous efforts made to demon- strate the fact of a retraction of the dendritic processes by histo- logical examinations of brains during sleep or narcosis have, how- ever, not been successful. 4. Anemia Theories of Sleep. — Numerous facts in physiology make it practically certain that during sleep there is a diminished * Pfliiger, "Archiv f. d. gesammte Physiologie," 10, 468, 1875. t Duval, " Comptes rendus de la soc. de biol.," February, 1895; and Cajal, "Archiv f. Anat. (u. Physiol.)/' 375, 1895. 246 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. flow of blood through the brain, a condition of cerebral anemia. In animals with the brain exposed or with a glass window in the skull it has been observed directly that the flow of blood to the cortex is diminished during sleep. Mosso's plethysmographic experi- ments mentioned above have been given a similar interpretation, and Tarchanoff's observations upon sleeping dogs, as well as direct determinations upon man by Brush and Fayerweather, show that the arterial pressure falls during sleep. Inasmuch as the lessened pressure in the arteries is accompanied by a dilatation of the vessels of the skin, as shown by the plethysmograph, it is probable, when the facts previously mentioned are taken into consideration, that the diminished pressure in the arteries forces less blood through the brain and more through the dilated vessels of the skin. In fact, as is explained in the section on circulation, it is probable that the blood-flow through the ■ brain is normally regulated in- directly by the circulation in other parts of the body. Constriction of blood-vessels elsewhere increases arterial pressure and shunts more blood through the brain, and vice versa. This general view is in accord with the fact that sensory stimuli and increased mental activity are accompanied by a constriction of the blood-vessels (of the skin) and a rise of arterial pressure, while, on the other hand, mental inactivity and especially sleep are accompanied by a dilatation of the blood-vessels of the body (skin vessels) and a fall of arterial pressure. All of our facts, therefore, point to an anemic condition of the brain during sleep, and some physiologists have believed that this condition precedes and causes the state of sleep, while others take the opposite view that it follows and is merely one result of sleep. On the basis of the plethysmographic experiments mentioned above the author* has proposed a theory of sleep in which the diminished flow of blood to the brain is ex- plained and is assumed to be the chief factor in bringing on sleep. The theory assumes that the periodicity of sleep is dependent mainly upon a rhythmical loss of tone in the vasomotor center in the medulla in consequence of fatigue from continued activity during the waking hours. That is, the vasomotor center is in constant action during this period; the continued flow of sensory stimuli and the constant activity of the brain act reflexly on this center and through it cause a constriction of the blood-vessels of the body, particularly of the skin, by means of which the blood- flow through the brain is maintained with an adequate velocity. In consequence of this varying but constant activity the center undergoes fatigue; stronger and stronger stimulation is necessary to maintain its normal tone, and eventually its effect on the blood- pressure becomes insufficient (to maintain an adequate flow through * Howell, " Journal of Experimental Medicine," 2, 313, 1897. THE PHYSIOLOGY OF SLEEP. 247 the brain and unconsciousness or sleep results, even against one's desires, as is shown by the experience of those who have attempted to keep awake much beyond the habitual period. Ordinarily, however, this fatigue of the vasomotor center and its resulting tendency to a cessation of activity is favored by our voluntary withdrawal of stimulation. Our preparations for sleep, closure of eyes, darkened and if possible quiet room, cessation from disturbing thoughts, result in a diminution of the sensory and mental stimuli that normally play upon the vasomotor center. The cessation of such stimuli may, indeed, at any time be all that is necessary to bring about a partial loss of activity in this center, a les- sened flow of blood through the brain, and a period of sleep which, however, is usually short. If, however, the vasomotor center has been previously fatigued, as may be supposed to be the case at the end of the day, the withdrawal of these stimuli permits it to fall into a more complete state of inactivity, and the diminution of blood-flow to the brain and the state of unconsciousness is longer lasting, — lasts indeed, according to the curves of which an example is given in Fig. 109, until the gradual resumption of activity in the vasomotor center brings about a constriction of the blood-vessels of the body and thus drives enough blood through the brain to cause spontaneous awakening. A third factor which must aid in the production of unconsciousness as a result of the lessened flow of blood, and in the return of consciousness in connection with the increased flow of blood, is the greater or less fatigue of the cortical cells themselves after a day's activity, and their greater irritability after a night's rest. Many factors, therefore, co-oper- ate in the development of the normal state of sleep lasting for six to eight hours out of twenty-four, but the central factor which explains its rapid onset, involving nearly simultaneously all the conscious areas of the brain, whether previously fatigued or not, and the equally sudden restoration to consciousness of the entire cortex, is to be found in the amount of blood-flow to the brain. Under normal conditions this is the factor that stands in most immediate relation to that appearance and disappearance of full consciousness which mark for us the limits of sleep. A similar view is advocated by Hill,* who believes, however, that the regu- lation of the blood-flow through the brain is effected through the vasomotor control of the splanchnic area, whereas the author's view is that the regulation is effected mainly through variations in the cutaneous circulation, — that is, for the normal occurrence of sleep. The drowsiness that follows a heavy meal is probably due mainly to the mechanical effect of a dilatation of the blood-vessels of * Hill, "The Physiology and Pathology of the Cerebral Circulation," Lon- don, 1896. 248 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. the viscera and the consequent diminution in the blood-flow through the brain; but the sleep that occurs at the end of the day is un- doubtedly connected with a dilatation of the blood-vessels of the skin of the trunk and extremities. What the condition in the vis- ceral organs may be at such times we have at present no means of knowing. Hypnotic Sleep. — The sleep that can be produced by so-called suggestion, the sleep of hypnotism, has been studied by means of the plethysmographic method.* The result, so far as the volume of the arm and hand is concerned, shows that in this con- dition, unlike normal sleep, there is a marked diminution in volume, and, therefore, we may believe, an increased constriction of the blood-vessels of the skin. This observation accords with the blanched appearance of the skin of the extremities, and with the statement that in deep hypnotic sleep the skin does not bleed readily when pricked with a needle. In view of our limited knowl- edge, however, it would be hazardous to base any comparison between normal and hypnotic sleep upon this single fact. * Walden, "American Journal of Physiology," 4, 124, 1900-1901. SECTION III. THE SPECIAL SENSES. CHAPTER XIV. CLASSIFICATION OF THE SENSES AND GENERAL STATEMENTS. Under the general term sense organ we may include not only the peripheral organ on which the stimulus acts, but also the sensory- nerve through which the impulses are conveyed to the center and the cortical center through which the reaction in consciousness is mediated. Classification of the Senses. — In general, we attempt to distinguish the action of the sense organs by the differences in their end reaction in consciousness. Each sense organ gives a different kind of response, the nature and distinctive features of which are recognized subjectively. The conscious sensations are said to differ in quality or modality. The qualitative difference in some cases is very distinct, — the difference between sensations of sound and of vision, for instance, — and on this subjective difference we base our efforts to give specific names to the sense organs con- cerned. This means of classification is not, however, applicable in all cases. While many of our sensations are so distinct in quality that we can recognize them and name them without difficulty, others are of a more obscure character. In addition to our sensa- tions of vision, hearing, smell, taste, pressure, temperature, and pain, there are doubtless many other sensations whose conscious reaction is less distinct in quality and for which our subjective means of recognition and classification are less satisfactory or entirely inadequate. Such, for instance, are the sensations from the muscles, from the semicircular canals and the vestibular sacs of the ear, and from many of the visceral organs. For the recogni- tion and classification of these senses and sense organs it is neces- sary to fall back upon the methods of anatomical and physiological analysis, methods which in many respects are uncertain. So also within the limits of any sensation of a given quality or modality, 249 250 THE SPECIAL SENSES. we distinguish certain subqualities. In vision we have many dif- ferent qualities which we designate by special names, — the series of different colors, for example. In sound sensations we distinguish different tones and different qualities of tones. But here, again, the subjective mark is often so indistinct in consciousness that it cannot be used satisfactorily for purposes of classifica- tion. In the odor sensations we distinguish many different quali- ties, each recognizable at the time that it is experienced, but their characteristics are so fugitive that heretofore it has not been possible to name them or group them in any satisfactory way. In studying the qualities of the various sensations so far as they are recognizable the effort of physiology has been to connect them with some definite anatomical or physiological peculiarity in the sense organs concerned. The final explanation of the differences in quality involves a study of the nature and properties of con- sciousness itself, — a subject which as yet has not been undertaken by physiology. At present wTe accept the fact of consciousness and the fact that there are different kinds or qualities of conscious- ness, and our investigations are directed only toward ascertaining the anatomical, physical, and chemical properties of the organs in- volved in the production of these subjective changes. In former times it was customary to divide the sensations into two different groups, — the special and the common senses, — the former including the so-called five senses of man, — namely, sight, hearing, touch, taste, and smell, — while under the latter were grouped all other sensations of less distinctive qualities. In physi- ology the belief that man has only five special senses has, however, long been abandoned. The sense of touch as ordinarily understood has been shown to consist of two or rather three distinct senses: pressure, heat and cold, and the sense of pain exhibited by the skin is in all essential respects as special and characteristic as those just named. There is, however, no certain standard as to what shall constitute a special in contradistinction to a common sense; so that a classification based on this nomenclature is unsatis- factory. In one respect, however, our senses show a difference which may be used as a basis for dividing them into two general groups. This difference lies in the manner of projection. We may assume that all of our sensations are aroused directly in the brain. In that organ take place the final changes which react in con- sciousness. But in no case are we conscious that this is the case. On the contrary, we project our sensations either to the exterior of the body or to some peripheral organ in the body, the effort being apparently to project it to the place where experi- ence has taught us that the acting stimulus arises. We may divide the senses, therefore, into two great groups: (1) The external CLASSIFICATION OF THE SENSES. 251 or rather the exterior senses, or those in which the sensations are projected to the exterior of the body, and which form, therefore the means through which we become acquainted with the outside world. The exterior senses include sight, hearing, taste, smell, pressure, and temperature (heat and cold). (2) The internal or interior senses, or those in which the sensations are projected to the interior of the body. It is through these senses that we acquire a knowledge of the condition of our body and perhaps also a knowl- edge of ourselves as an existence or organism distinct from the ex- ternal world. Among the interior senses we must include pain, muscle sense, the sensations from the semicircular canals and ves- tibule of the internal ear, hunger, thirst, sexual sense, fatigue, and in addition perhaps other less definite sensations from the visceral organs. This line of demarcation, although it holds so well in most cases, is not absolutely distinctive. The temperature sense, for instance, is, so to speak, on the border line between the two groups; we may project this sensation either to the exterior or to the interior according to circumstances. When the temperature nerves are excited simultaneously with the pressure nerves, we project the sensation to the exterior, to the stimulating body. If the skin is touched by a hot or cold solid object we speak of the object as being hot or cold. If, however, the same nerves are stimulated by warm gases or even liquids under conditions that do not involve the pressure sense we refer the change to ourselves, — we are hot or cold, as the case may be. So also when the skin is heated by the blood the resulting sensation is projected to the skin. It would seem that the habit of projection is acquired by experience, and that those senses whose organs are habitually affected by objects from without we learn to project to the object giving rise to the stimulus. The Doctrine of Specific Nerve Energies. — The term specific nerve energy we owe to Johannes Muller (1801-1858). The term is in some respects unfortunate, as at present in the physical sci- ences the word energy is used to designate certain specific properties of matter. The phrase specific nerve energy in physiology, however, is intended to designate the fact that each sensory unit arouses or mediates its own specific quality of sensation, the specific energy of the optic apparatus being visual sensations, of the auditory apparatus sound sensations, etc., and each sensory nerve or apparatus can give no other than its own quality of sensation. Whether this specificity in the reaction of each sensory nerve is due to some pecu- liarity in the nerve itself or to a peculiarity of the part of the brain in which it terminates Muller left an open question, although he called attention to the fact that the central ending is capable of giving its specific effect in consciousness independently of the con- 252 THE SPECIAL SENSES. ducting nerve fibers. With regard to this latter question the opinions of physiologists still differ. Most physiologists, perhaps, adopt the view that the specific reaction in consciousness is due to the central ending, — that, in other words, the different sensory parts of the cortex give different kinds or qualities of consciousness, while the sensory nerve fibers are simply conductors of nerve im- pulses, which, however much they may differ in intensity, are qualitatively the same in all nerve fibers. According to this view, it would result, as du Bois-Reymond expressed it, that, if the auditory nerve fibers were attached to the visual center and the optic fibers to the auditory center, we would see the thunder and hear the lightning. Each typical sense-organ from this standpoint consists of three essential parts: the central ending, which deter- mines the quality of the sensation; the peripheral end-organ, retina, cochlea, etc., which determines whether or not any given form of stimulus shall be effective and which in most cases is con- structed so as to be responsive to a special form of stimulus desig- nated as its adequate stimulus; and of connecting neurons whose only function is to conduct the nerve impulses originating in the end-organ. The fact, therefore, that the light waves can stimulate the rods and cones of the retina, but are an inadequate stimulus probably to the hair cells of the cochlea or the taste buds of the tongue, is due to a peculiarity in structure of the rods and cones; but the fact that the impulses conducted by the optic fibers arouse a peculiar modality of sensation is not due to any peculiarity in structure in these fibers or in the rods and cones, but to a charac- teristic structure of the optic centers. The positive experimental evidence for the correctness of this view is not conclusive, but, on the whole, is impressive. Such facts as the following may be noted : 1. When sensory nerve fibers are stimulated otherwise than through their end-organs each reacts, if it reacts at all, according to its specific energy, — that is, it produces its own quality of sensa- tion. When the optic nerve is cut, for instance, the mechanical stimulus causes a flash of light ; when the chorda tympani is stimu- lated in the tympanic cavity by mechanical, electrical, or chemical stimuli sensations of taste are aroused. 2. Mechanical pressure upon the peripheral nerves distributed to the skin may cause a loss of some of the cutaneous senses in certain areas of the skin with a retention of others. Thus the senses of pressure and temperature may be lost and that of pain retained, or pain may be lost and pressure retained. Such facts agree with the view that each sense has its own set of nerve fibers ; those that mediate pain can not by a mere modification of the stimulus give also a sense of pressure. 3. The only objective manifestation of a nerve impulse that we CLASSIFICATION OF THE SENSES. 253 can study in the nerve itself is the electrical change that accom- panies it or that perhaps constitutes its essence. This electrical change is qualitatively the same in all kinds of nerve fibers, and this fact agrees with the view that the nerve impulse is qualitatively the same in all fibers. So far as the sensory nerve fibers are concerned, the chief ob- jection to this view of the doctrine of specific nerve energies is found perhaps in the difficulty or impossibility of applying it to the explanation of color vision. According to the strict interpreta- tion of the view, each fundamental color sense, being distinct in quality, should be mediated by its own set of nerve fibers. When Helmholtz first formulated his theory of color vision he spoke, therefore, of three kinds of nerve fibers, — the red, the green, and the violet, — each when stimulated alone giving its own specific sensation and not capable of giving any other. The facts accumu- lated regarding color vision, however, seem to show that this view will not hold. One and the same cone, with its connecting fiber, may give rise to any or all of the primary color sensations, and, unless we choose to further subdivide the nerve unit and assume that the separate nerve fibrils of which the axis cylinder is composed constitute the separate conductors for the primary sense qualities, it would seem to be impossible to apply the doctrine of specific energies to this case. Not too much weight should be given per- haps to this objection. For it must be remembered that all of our present theories of color vision are unsatisfactory, and possibly when we attain to the right point of view the facts may not be so difficult to interpret in terms of this theory of specific energies. The alternative view proposed in place of the doctrine of specific nerve energies assumes that the nerve impulses may vary in quality as well as in intensity, and that therefore one and the same nerve fiber may arouse different qualities of sensation, and have different end effects according to the character of the impulse conveyed. This point of view is not capable of much discussion, since there are no positive facts that support it. It is logically satisfactory in meeting the cases in which the former view seems to be unsatisfactory. It is difficult, however, in our ignorance of the nature of the nerve impulse to imagine in what respects it may possibly differ in character. The Weber-Fechner (Psychophysical) Law. — One difficulty that has been encountered in the physiological study of sensory nerves is that the end reaction cannot be measured with exactness. With efferent nerves the end reaction is a contraction or secretion that can be estimated quantitatively in terms of our physical and chemical units of measurement. But the end reaction of a sensory nerve is a state of consciousness for which we have no standard of 254 THE SPECIAL SENSES. measurement. Weber, in studying the relation between the strength of the stimulus and the amount of the resulting sensation, availed himself of the method of the least detectible change in sen- sation ; that is, he determined the increase in stimulus at different levels necessary to cause a just perceptible increase in the sensation. By means of this method he arrived at the significant result that the increase in stimulus necessary to cause this change is, within physiological limits, a definite fractional increment of the acting stimulus. If, for instance, with a weight of 30 gms. upon the finger it requires an increment of -fa— that is, one additional gram— to make a just perceptible difference in the pressure sensa- ^~ ,-- ~n -i ^» -'' "" h 3 *~' *" 70. 10 ,' 7- sro s y .63 • * V / / 3.1 6 i / z. 37 77 i . U.3 3 / / 1 6 / . Z J 1. lA f i r c ' b 5 ' A 7 / f /. z / 5 J' \ /. 5- It \ i / /8 EXCITATION X. Fig. 111. — Curve to indicate the Weber-Fechner law of a logarithmical relation be- tween excitation and sensation. — (From Waller.) The excitations are indicated along the abscissas, the sensations along the ordinates. The increase in sensation is represented as tak- ing place in equal steps, "the minimal perceptible difference," while the corresponding excitations require an increasing increment of i at each step, namely 1, 1.33, 1.77, 2.37, etc. That is, for equal increments of sensation increasing increments of stimulation are necessary. tion; then, with a weight of 60 gms. upon the finger the addition of another gram would not be perceived ; it would require again an increment of -3V — that is, 2 gms. — to make a just perceptible dif- ference in sensation. This relationship is known as Weber's law. While its exactness has often been disputed, it seems to be generally 'admitted that for a median range of stimulation the law expresses Ithe approximate relation between the two variables considered. jFechner attempted to give this law a more quantitative and ex- pensive application by assuming that just perceptible differences in sensation represent actually equal amounts of sensation. Ac- cepting this assumption, we can express the relationship between stimulus and sensation as determined by Weber's experiments by CLASSIFICATION OF THE SENSES. 255 saying that for the sensation to increase by equal amounts, — that is, by arithmetical progression, — the stimulus must vary according to a certain factor, — that is, by geometrical progression. The sensation may be regarded as a geometrical function of the stimulus. If the relation between stimulus and sensation is repre- sented as a curve in which the ordinates express the sensation in- creasing by equal amounts, and the abscissas the corresponding stimuli increasing at each interval by J, a result is obtained such as is represented in the accompanying figure (Fig. 111). A curve of this kind is a logarithmical curve, and Fechner expressed the rela- tionship between stimulus and sensation in what has been called the psychophysical law, — namely, that the sensation varies as the logarithm of the stimulus. From the physiological standpoint it is important to bear in mind, as has been emphasized by Waller,* that several steps intervene between the action of the external stimulus and the production of the conscious sensation. The external stim- ulus acts first on the end-organ, this in turn upon the sensory nerve fiber, producing a nerve impulse which finally in the brain gives the conscious reaction. It is a question, therefore, whether the logarith- mical relation of the stimulus holds between it and the reaction of the end-organ or between the internal stimulus — that is, the sensory nerve impulse — and the psychical reaction. This author has given some facts obtained by recording the action current in the optic nerve, the retina being stimulated by known intensities of light, which indicate that the relation observed is between the external stimulus and the internal stimulus, — that is, the sensory nerve impulse. * Waller, "Brain," 201, 1895. CHAPTER XV. CUTANEOUS AND INTERNAL SENSATIONS. According to the older views, the sensory nerves of the skin give sensations of touch. Modern physiology has shown, however, that these nerves mediate at least four different qualities of sensation, — namely, pressure, warmth, cold, and pain. Our so-called touch sensations are usually compound, consisting of a pressure and a temperature component and also very frequently an element of muscle sense when muscular efforts are involved, as, for instance, in measuring weights or resistances. The four sensory qualities enu- merated constitute the cutaneous senses, and they are present, or, to speak more accurately, the nerves through which these senses are mediated are present not only over the general cutaneous surface, but also in those membranes — such as the mucous membrane of the mouth and the rectum (stomodeum and proctodeum) — which embryologically are formed from the epiblast. The surfaces in the interior of the body, on the contrary, — such as the membranes of the alimentary canal, muscles, fascia?, etc., — have only nerves of pain, but no sense of touch or temperature. Of these cutaneous senses, three — pressure, warmth, and cold — may be grouped with the exterior senses, the sensations being projected to the exterior of the body, into the substance causing the sensation. Although, as was mentioned above, the temperature sensations under conditions — fever, vascular dilatation, etc. — may be projected to parts of the skin itself and be felt as changes in ourselves. The temperature sensations are, in fact, projected to the exterior whenever they are combined with pressure sensations, the latter serving, as it were, as the dominant sense. The pain sense, on the other hand, belongs to the group of interior senses, the sensations being always projected into our own body and being felt as changes in ourselves. The Punctiform Distribution of the Cutaneous Senses. — A most interesting fact in regard to the cutaneous senses is that they are not distributed uniformly over the whole skin, but are present in discrete points or spots. This fact was first clearly established by Blix,* although it was discovered independently * Blix, "Zeitschrift f. Biologie," 20, 141, 1884; Donaldson, "Mind," 39 1, 1885. See also Goldscheider, "Archiv f. Physiologie," 1885, suppl. volume. 256 CUTANEOUS AND INTERNAL SENSATIONS. 257 by Goldscheider and in this country by Donaldson. These ob- servers paid attention chiefly to the warm and cold spots. The existence of these spots may be demonstrated easily by anyone upon himself by moving a metallic point gently over the skin. If the point has a temperature below that of the skin it will be noticed that at certain spots it arouses simply a feeling of contact or pressure, while at other spots it gives a distinct sensation of coldness. If, on the other hand, the point is warmer than the skin it will at certain spots give a sensation of warmth. On mark- » » 'r * Fig. 112. — Representation of the distribution of cold and warm spots on the volar surface of forearm in a space 2 cms. by 4 cms. The red dots represent the cold spots as tested at a temperature of 10° C. The black dots represent the warm spots as tested at a temperature of 41° to 48° C. ing the cold and warm spots thus obtained it is found that they occupy different positions on the skin. Elaborate charts have been made of the warm and cold spots on different regions of the skin, the apparatus usually employed being a metal tube through which water of any desired temperature may be circulated. The temperature of the skin, whatever it may be, forms the zero line; any object of a higher temperature stimulates only the warm spots, while one of a lower temperature acts upon the cold spots. The pressure sense and the pain sense are also distributed in a punctiform manner; they have been studied most 17 258 THE SPECIAL SENSES. carefully by von Frey.* To determine the location of the pressure points he used fine hairs of different diameters fastened to a wooden handle. The cross-areas of these hairs are determined by measure- ments under the microscope, and the pressure exerted by each is measured by pressing it upon the scale pan of a balance. The quotient of the pressure exerted divided by the cross-area of the hair in square millimeters, ^"^-2, reduces the pressure to a uniform unit of area. For the pain points fine needles may be employed or stiff hairs similar to those used for the pressure points. From the experiments made there seems to be no doubt that each of the four cutaneous senses has its own spots of distribution in the skin, those for pain being most numerous and those for warmth the least numerous. There is some reason for believing also that the nerve endings mediating the pain sense lie most superficially in the skin and those for the warm sense the deepest. Specific Nerve Energies of the Cutaneous Nerves. — Many attempts have been made to determine whether the doctrine of specific nerve energies applies to these cutaneous senses; that is, whether each sense has its own nerve fibers capable of giving only its own quality of sensation. The evidence, on the whole, is favorable to this view. According to some observers, electrical or mechanical stimulation of the different points calls forth for each its character- istic reaction. Donaldson has found that cocain applied to the eye or throat destroys the senses of pain and pressure, but leaves those of heat and cold, which again supports the view of separate fibers for each sense. In addition there are a number of interesting pathological cases which point in the same direction. In some lesions of the cord — syringomyelia, for instance — the senses in the skin of the parts below are dissociated, — that is, there may be loss of pain and temperature in a certain area with a retention of the pressure sense. — a fact which indicates that these senses have separate paths and therefore separate nerve fibers. Still more interesting cases of dissociation are reported as the result of the compression of peripheral nerve trunks. Thus, Barker f describes his own case, in which, as the result of the pressure of a cervical rib upon some of the cords of the brachial plexus, there was a region in the arm lacking in the pressure and temperature senses, but retain- ing the sense of pain. He quotes other cases in which the reverse dissociation occurred, pressure sense alone remaining. The simplest explanation of these facts is the view that each pressure, pain, warm, and cold spot is supplied by its own nerve fiber, and that each, when stimulated, reacts, if it reacts at all, only with its own * Von Frey, "Konigl. Sachsischen Gesellschaft der Wissenschaften. Math.- phys. Klasse," 1894-95-96. t Barker, "Journal of Experimental Medicine," 1, 348, 1896. CUTANEOUS AND INTERNAL SENSATIONS. 259 peculiar quality of sensation. According to this view, artificial stimulation, if properly controlled, of the trunks of the nerves supplying the skin should be capable of bringing out these different sense qualities. Experiments made with this point in view have not, however, been very successful. Mechanical or electrical stimu- lation of the ulnar nerve, for instance, gives usually only pain sensa- tions, although if the stimulus is feeble contact sensations are aroused. The method, however, is probably at fault. In the case of amputated fingers or limbs a more decisive result is obtained. As is well known, individuals after such operations may for many years have sensations of their lost fingers or limbs. In such cases the pressure in the stump of the wound acting upon the central ends of the sensory fibers arouses sensations which are projected in the usual way, and give the feeling that would be experienced if the lost parts were still there and were stimulated in the normal manner. The Temperature Senses. — The main facts regarding the distribution of heat and cold spots have been determined. In gen- eral, the cold spots are more numerous than the warm spots, and react more promptly to their adequate stimulus. The cold spots or the cold sense may be present in places devoid of the sense of warmth; thus, it is said that the glans penis possesses only the cold sense. The threshold stimulus varies also in different parts of the skin, the tip of the tongue requiring the smallest stimulus to arouse a sensation, and the eyelids, forehead, cheeks, lips, limbs, and trunk following in the order named. According to Goldscheider, the spots on most portions of the skin form chains that have a some- what radiate arrangement with reference to the hair follicles. The temperature points possess each its adequate stimulus, that for the cold spot being temperatures lower than the skin or of the terminal organ of the cold nerves, that for the heat spots temperatures higher than their own. Apparently, therefore, one end-organ is excited by a diminution in the atomic movements of its organ, and the other by an increase. Nothing is known, however, of the exact nature of the stimulating process. From the standpoint of specific nerve energies it is most interesting to find that these points, particu- larly the cold spots, may be stimulated by other than their adequate stimuli. Mechanical and electrical stimulation has in the hands of several observers been efficient in causing a sensation of cold upon a cold spot and of heat upon a warm spot. Some chemical stimuli are also effective. Menthol applied to the skin gives a cold sensa- tion, while, on the other hand, if the arm be plunged into a jar of carbon-dioxid gas a distinct warm sensation will be experienced. A curious effect of this kind is what is known as the paradoxical cold reaction. It is produced by applying a very warm object, with a 260 THE SPECIAL SENSES. temperature of 40° to 60° C, to a cold spot. In many cases this spot is stimulated and a cold sensation is felt. The same result may be felt at the instant of entering a hot bath. Many efforts have been made to determine whether there is a specific kind of end-organ for each of these senses. Numerous observers have cut out the skin from cold or hot spots and examined the removed part carefully by histological methods. The general result has been that no distinctive end-organs have been found. Von Frey, how- ever, believes that, although the heat spots are supplied simply by a terminal end plexus, the cold spots in some places at least have as a special end-organ the end-bulbs of Krause. This conclusion is based upon the fact that these end-bulbs are found in places, such as the glans penis and conjunctiva, where the cold sense is espe- cially prominent or exclusively present. The Sense of Pressure. — The pressure points are smaller and more numerous than the cold or warm spots. Von Frey has shown that in those portions of the body that are supplied with hairs the pressure points lie over the hair follicles. The pressure nerve fibers, in fact, terminate in a ring surrounding the hair follicle, this form of termination serving as an end-organ. On account of their position they are stimulated by any pressure exerted upon the hair. The hair, indeed, acts like a lever and transmits any pres- sure applied to it with increased intensity, acting, therefore, as re- gards the pressure organ somewhat like the ear-bones in the case of the endings of the auditory nerve. In parts of the body not furnished with hairs the tactile or Meissner corpuscles are found and these structures doubtless function as pressure end-organs. They are particularly abundant in the parts of the hand and feet in which a delicate sense of pressure is present in spite of a much thick- ened epidermis. It has been estimated that for the entire surface of the body, excluding the head region, there are about 500,000 of these pressure points. These points are close together on those parts, such as the tongue and fingers, which have delicate a tactile sense and more widely scattered where the sense is less developed. The Threshold Stimulus and the Localizing Power. — The delicacy of the sense of pressure may be measured by determining the minimal pressure necessary to arouse a sensation, — that is, the threshold stimulus, — or it may be estimated in terms of the power of discriminating two contiguous stimuli, — that is, the mini- mal distance that two points must be apart in order for the sensa- tions to be recognized as distinct. The two methods of measure- ment do not coincide. As determined by the threshold stimulus, the greatest delicacy is exhibited by the skin of the face, the fore- head, and temples. According to the older methods of measure- ment, the forehead will perceive a pressure of 2 mgs., while the skin CUTANEOUS AND INTERNAL SENSATIONS. 261 of the tips of the fingers needs a pressure of from 5 to 15 mgs. to arouse a perceptible sensation. The back of the hand or the arm is more sensitive from this standpoint than the tips of the fingers. When measured by the power of discriminating two points — that is, the localizing sense — the tips of the fingers are far more sensitive than the skin of the face or of the arm. This latter prop- erty, in fact, stands in relation to the closeness of the pressure points to one another. The localizing sense may be determined by Weber's method of using a pair of compasses with blunt points. For any given area of the skin the power of discrimination or local- ization is expressed in terms of the number of millimeters between the two points at which they are just distinguished as two separate sensations when applied simultaneously to the skin. Instruments made for this purpose are designated as esthesiometers. They carry two points the distance of which apart can be readily adjusted and read off on a scale. The most satisfactory form of esthesiom- eter is that devised by von Frey. The two points in this case are made by long, rather stiff hairs whose pressure can be made quite uniform. According to the older measurements, the localizing sense of different parts of the skin varies greatly, as is shown by the accompanying table: Tip of the tongue 1.1 mms. Tip of finger, palmar surface > . . . . 2.3 " Second phalanx finger, palmar surface 4.5 " First phalanx finger, palmar surface 5.5 " Third phalanx finger, dorsal surface 6.8 " Middle of palm 8 to 9 " Second phalanx finger, dorsal surface 11.3 " Forehead 22.6 " Back of the hand. 31.6 " Forearm 40.6 " Sternum 45 " Along the spine 54 " Middle of neck or back 67.7 " The tips of the tongue and the fingers are, therefore, the most delicate surfaces, and that the tongue surpasses the fingers in this respect is easily within the experience of everyone who will recall the ease with which small objects between the teeth are detected by the tongue as compared with the fingers. From the above data it is evident also that the whole skin may be imagined as composed of a mosaic of areas of different sizes, the sensory circles of Weber, in each of which two or more simultaneous stimulations of the pres- sure nerves give only one pressure sensation. The size of these areas, particularly where they are large, may be reduced by practice, as is shown by the increased tactile sensibility of the blind. The fact that we can recognize two simultaneous pressure stimuli of the skin as two distinct sensations implies that the two sensations have 262 THE SPECIAL SENSES. some recognizable difference in consciousness. This difference is spoken of as the local sign. We may believe that every sensitive point upon the skin has its own distinctive local sign or quality, and that by experience we have learned to project each local sign more or less accurately to its proper place on the skin surface. Two points on this surface that are a great distance apart are easily recognized as different ; but as we bring the points closer together the difference becomes less marked and finally disappears when the distance corresponds to the area of the sensory circle for the part of the skin investigated, for instance, 1 mm. for the tongue, 22 mms. for the forehead, etc. The ultimate limit of the power of discrimination was assumed by Weber to depend upon the area of distribution of a single nerve fiber. Assuming that each nerve fiber at its termi- nation spreads over a certain skin area, it was suggested that the size of this area forms a limit to the power of discrimination, since two stimuli within it would affect a single fiber and therefore would give a single sensation. This view, however, has not been supposed to accord with the facts even when the additional supposition was made that the local signs of two adjacent fibers may not be distinct enough for us to recognize them as separate and that practically there must be a number of intervening unstimulated areas, the number varying according to the sensitiveness of the area. Von Frey has, however, given a new method of testing the localizing sense of the skin, the results of which seem to accord with this anatomical explanation. If instead of applying the two points simultaneously they are applied in succession, at an interval of one second, the individual can distinguish the difference when two neighboring pressure points are stimulated. Each pressure point in the skin, therefore, has a local sign, which enables us to distinguish it from all others, and by this method the ultimate sensory circles on the skin become much smaller than when measured by the usual method of Weber. The center of each is a pressure point and the area is determined by the distance from this center at which an isolated stimulation of this point can be obtained. It seems probable, moreover, that each of these pressure points is connected to the brain by a separate nerve path, possibly a single fiber, and that this anatomical arrangement determines the limitation of the localizing sense for different regions of the skin. The Pain Sense. — Pain is probably the sense that is most widely distributed in the body. It is present throughout the skin, and under certain conditions may be aroused by stimulation of sensory nerves in the various visceral organs, and indeed in all of the mem- branes of the body. Our knowledge of the physiological properties of the end-organs and nerves mediating this sense is chiefly limited CUTANEOUS AND INTERNAL SENSATIONS. 263 to the skin, and for cutaneous pain at least the evidence, as stated above, is very strongly in favor of the view that there exists a special set of fibers which have a specific energy for pain. All recent ob- servers agree that the pain sense has a punctiform distribution in the skin, the pain points being even more numerous than the pres- sure points. The threshold stimulus of these points in various regions may be determined by von Frey's stimulating hairs, and experiments of this kind show, as we should expect, that it varies greatly. The cornea, for instance, gives sensations of pain with much weaker stimuli than in the case of the finger tips. In general, however, the threshold stimulus is much higher for the pain than for the pressure points. Histological examination of the pain points indicates that there is no special end-organ, the stimulus taking effect upon the free endings of the nerve fibers. Any of the usual forms of artificial nerve stimuli may affect these endings if of suf- ficient intensity, and, as is well known, stimuli applied to sensory nerve trunks affect these fibers with especial ease. A temperature of 50° to 70° C. applied to an afferent nerve will cause violent pain sensations, but has no effect upon the motor nerve fibers in the same trunk. Mechanical stimulation gives usually only pain sensations, and the results of inflammatory changes, as in neuritis or neuralgia, are equally marked. Localization or Projection of Pain Sensations. — Under normal conditions cutaneous pains are projected with accuracy to the point stimulated, and it is possible that this result is due in part at least to the training acquired in connection with concomitant pressure stimuli, the latter acting as a guide or aid in the projection. Thus in the cases referred to above, in which a portion of the skin had lost the sense of pressure and temperature, but retained that of pain, it was found that the localization was very incomplete. Pain arising in the internal organs, on the contrary, is located very inaccurately. The pain from a severe toothache, for example, may be projected quite diffusely to the side of the face. A very interesting fact in this connection is that such pains are often referred to points on the skin and may be accompanied by skin areas of tenderness. Pains of this kind that are misref erred to the surface of the body are desig- nated as reflected pains. It has been shown by Head* and others that the different visceral organs have, in this respect, a more or less definite relation to certain areas of the skin. Pains arising from stimuli acting upon the intestines are located in the skin of the back, loins, and abdomen in the area supplied by the ninth, tenth, and eleventh dorsal spinal nerves ; pains from irritations in the stomach are located in the skin over the ensiform cartilage ; those from the heart in the scapular region, and so on. The explanation offered * Head, "Brain," 16, 1, 1893, and 24, 345, 1901. 264 THE SPECIAL SENSES. for this misreference is that the pain is referred to the skin region that is supplied from the spinal segment from which the organ in question receives its sensory fibers, the misreference being due to a diffusion in the nerve centers. As Head expresses it, "when a painful stimulus is applied to a part of low sensibility in close central connection with a part of much greater sensibility the pain produced is felt in the part of higher sensibility rather than in the part of lower sensibility to which the stimulus was actually applied.' ' It is interesting that affections of the serous cavities — e.g., the peritoneum — do not cause reflected pains or cutaneous tenderness as in the case of the viscera. Another notable fact in this connection is the occurrence of the condition known as allochiria. When from any cause one or other of the cutaneous senses is depressed in a given area stimulation in this region may give sensations which are re- ferred to the symmetrical area on the other side of the body, or, if this also is involved, it may be referred to the area next above or below in the spinal order. The above law, according to which pro- jection is made to the area of higher sensibility most closely con- nected with the area of low sensibility, seems to hold in this case also. The Muscle Sense. — The existence of a special set of sensory nerve fibers distributed to the muscles was clearly recognized by some of the older physiologists. Charles Bell,* for example, says: "Between the brain and the muscles there is a circle of nerves; one nerve conveys the influence from the brain to the muscle; another gives the sense of the condition of the muscle to the brain." The conclusive proof of the existence of such fibers, however, has only been furnished within recent years. It has been demonstrated that there are special sensory endings in the muscles, the so-called muscle spindles, and in the attached tendons, the tendon spindles or tendon organs of Golgi. The muscle spindles are found most frequently in the neighborhood of the tendons, at tendinous inter- sections or under aponeuroses. Sherrington! has shown that the nerve fibers in them do not degenerate after section of the anterior roots of the corresponding spinal nerves and are therefore derived from the posterior roots. In the muscles of the limbs he estimates that from one-half to one-third of the fibers in the muscular nerve branches are sensory, and that most of these sensory fibers end in the muscle spindles. On the physiological and clinical side facts of various kinds have accumulated that make clear the existence of this group of sensory fibers and emphasize their essential importance in the co-ordination of our muscular movements. It has been shown * Bell, "The Nervous System of the Human Body," third edition, Lon- don, 1844, p. 200. f Sherrington, "Journal of Physiology," 17, 237, 1894. CUTANEOUS AND INTERNAL SENSATIONS. 265 that stimulation of the nerves distributed to the muscles or mechani- cal stimulation of the muscles themselves causes a depressor effect upon blood-pressure, thus demonstrating the presence of afferent fibers in the muscles. As described in the section upon the central nervous system, the numerous experiments upon the effect of section of the posterior and lateral columns of the cord and observations upon the results of pathological lesions of the posterior columns (tabes dorsalis) give results which are interpreted to mean that fibers of muscular sensibility form the most important group in the posterior columns and constitute, as well, perhaps, the long, ascend- ing fibers in the tracts of Flechsig and Gowers in the lateral col- umns. It is believed, therefore, that our so-called voluntary muscles are richly supplied with afferent fibers and that the impulses carried by these fibers to the brain are necessary for the proper contraction of the muscles and particularly for the adequate combination of the contractions of groups of muscles in the co-ordinated movements of equilibrium. Indeed, section of the posterior roots of the spinal nerves supplying a given region is followed by a loss of control of the muscles in this region hardly less complete than the paralysis produced by direct section of the anterior roots; the muscles not only lose their tonicity in consequence of the dropping out of the reflex sensory stimuli from the skin and muscles of the region, but they are apparently withdrawn from voluntary control in spite of the maintenance of their normal motor connections. Within the central nervous system the fibers of muscle sense are traced into the nuclei of Goll and of Burdach in the medulla and thence partly into the cerebellum and partly into the cerebrum by way of the median fillet. Within the cerebrum they end in the cortex of the parietal lobe in the region of the posterior central convolution. There is reason to believe that this cortical sense area of the muscle sense is connected by association fibers with the motor areas lying anterior to the fissure of Rolando, and we have thus a reflex arc, — or, as Bell expressed it, a circle of nerves between the muscles and the brain. It is probable that a similar arc or circle is formed by the connections through the cerebellum, and still a third one of a lower order by the connections in the spinal cord. In the higher animals the impulses received in the cerebellum through the fibers of muscle sense, in connection with those received from the semicircular canals and vestibular sacs of the ear, furnish the sensory basis for the cerebellar control of muscular movements, particularly of the synergetic combination necessary in locomotion. Through the circle or arc in the cortex of the cerebrum it may be supposed that our characteristic voluntary movements are effected, and it may be doubted whether a so-called voluntary contraction can be made when this circle is broken on the sensory side. Whether or not this 266 THE SPECIAL SENSES. latter suggestion is true it seems to be beyond doubt that adequately- controlled voluntary movements depend for their adaptation upon the inflow of sensory impulses along the fibers of muscle sense. We have a certain consciousness of the condition of our muscles at all times, and if we were deprived of this knowledge we should be unable to control them properly, perhaps unable to use them voluntarily. The Quality of the Muscle Sense. — Our conscious realization of muscular sensibility is not distinct. Under ordinary conditions the untrained person is unaware of the presence of such a sense; but physiological analysis enables us to realize its existence. What we designate as the feeling of resistance and of weight depends usually partly upon the pressure sense, but largely upon the muscle sense. In estimating the difference in weight between two bodies our judgment is much more exact if the bodies are lifted by muscular effort, as is our custom, than if they are simply allowed to press upon the skin; and in all calculations of resistance to effort it is the amount of muscular contraction exerted that furnishes us with the chief sensory basis for our judgments. So also in the judg- ments of distance based upon visual impressions it is believed that for close objects, particularly, the muscle sense connected with the extrinsic and intrinsic musculature of the eyeballs plays a funda- mental part. Doubtless also this sense takes an essential part in the primitive formation of our conceptions of space, since it may be assumed that the continual movements of the extremities in connection with our visual and tactile impressions furnish essen- tial data upon which we build our perceptions of distance and size, our judgments of spatial relations. As is explained in the chapter on the physiology of the ear, the sensations from the semi- circular canals and vestibular sacs co-operate in giving data for these fundamental conceptions, and it is not possible for us to disentangle the parts taken by these senses separately in building up our knowledge of the external world. The muscle sense is reckoned usually among the internal (or common) senses, — that is, those which are projected to the interior of the body and are felt as changes in ourselves. A little reflection, however, demonstrates that, like the temperature sense, it may under conditions be pro- jected to the exterior and be interpreted as a quality of external objects. Weight and resistance, for example, are attributed to the objects giving rise to the feeling of muscular effort, and it may be said, perhaps, that, as in the case of temperature, the feeling is projected more or less clearly to the exterior when it is combined with the pressure sense which acts as the predominating or guiding factor in the projection. In excessive muscular effort the quality of the muscle sensation undergoes a change and becomes strong CUTANEOUS AND INTERNAL SENSATIONS. 267 enough to make a distinct and peculiar impression upon our con- sciousness. We designate this feeling as fatigue, but there is no question apparently that this sensation is mediated through the same nerve fibers that ordinarily give us our muscular sensibility. Sensations of Hunger and Thirst. — Hunger and thirst are typical interior (or common) sensations. We feel them as changes in ourselves. Neither sense has been the direct object of much experimental investigation, and what knowledge we possess is there- fore derived largely from accidental or pathological sources. Hunger in its mild form is designated as appetite. It occurs normally at a certain interval after meals, and is referred or projected more or less accurately to the stomach. It is not known whether this sense is mediated by a special set of sensory fibers distributed to the mucous membrane of the stomach, or whether, perhaps, it may be a quality of the sensory impressions from the muscular coat. The former view seems more probable, especially when it is remembered that loss of appetite or anorexia is so frequently an accompaniment of pathological changes in the membrane of the stomach. The nervous mechanism through which this sense is me- diated is of most essential importance and deserves more careful study at the hands of physiologists and pathologists. Under ordi- nary conditions of life all of the regulation of the amount and quality of the food necessary to the proper nutrition of the body and the maintenance of body equilibrium is effected through this sense. Its striking influence upon the body at large is well illustrated in the case of animals (pigeons, dogs) deprived of their cerebrum. During the period of fasting these animals show all the external signs of hunger and keep in continual, restless movement that seems to imply a con- stantly acting sensory stimulus. We may assume that appetite has its sensory origin, its peripheral nerve endings in the stomach, and that these endings are excited in some unknown way when the stomach is empty. This gastric hunger, as it might be called, disappears, or the appetite is appeased when the stomach is filled. This fact in itself would indicate that the stimulus has a local origin in the stomach, and is not dependent upon any general change in the nutritive condition of the body. The appetite is satisfied by filling the stomach with food long before this food is actually absorbed and distributed to the tissues. The inges- tion of totally indigestible material would probably have temporarily a similar result. The exact nature of the conditions that lead to or cause a stimulation of the sensory nerves of appetite in the stomach remains unexplained. The well-known fact that muscular exercise and low temperatures and particularly a combination of the two cause a marked augmentation of the appetite would suggest that the sensory stimulus is influenced by the extent or character 268 THE SPECIAL SENSES. of the oxidations in the muscular tissues, and that, therefore, some substance may be formed as the result of these oxidations which affects the sensory nerves of the stomach. The same general sug- gestion is contained in the fact that diabetics exhibit an abnormal appetite in spite of abundant feeding. In these individuals the carbohydrate food escapes oxidation more or less completely, and the metabolism, particularly in the muscles, involves, therefore, to a greater extent, the oxidation of proteid material, — a fact which may stand in some relation to the abnormal appetite that is observed. The complexity of the nervous apparatus that controls the the appe- tite is shown also by many facts from the experiences of life and from the results of laboratory investigations. For example, it is found that large amounts of gelatin in the diet, although at first accepted willingly, soon provoke a feeling of dislike and aversion to this particular foodstuff such as cannot be overcome. An animal will starve rather than use the gelatin, although all of our direct physio- logical evidence would indicate that this substance is an efficient food, playing much the same part as the fats or carbohydrates. A fact of this kind indicates that the sensory apparatus of the appe- tite is influenced in some specific way by the metabolism of this particular material. So also the feeling of satiety and aversion for food that follows overfeeding indicates something more than a sim- ple removal of the sensations of appetite ; it implies an active state, due possibly to the excitation of sensory fibers of a different char- acter. With regard to the effects of prolonged starvation, the pangs of hunger that are felt at first do not seem to increase in in- tensity to such an extent as to cause actual suffering. The testi- mony of the " professional fasters," at least, seems to show that, if water is provided, prolonged deprivation of food is not accompanied by the intense discomfort or suffering popularly associated with the idea of complete starvation. The Sense of Thirst. — Our sensations of thirst are projected more or less accurately to the pharynx, and the facts that we know would seem to indicate that the sensory nerves of this region have the important function of mediating this sense. The water con- tents of the body are subject to great changes. Through the lungs, the skin, and the kidneys water is lost continually in amounts that vary with the conditions of life. This loss affects the blood directly, but is doubtless made good, so far as this tissue is concerned, by a call upon the great mass of water contained in the storehouse of the tissues. To restore the body tissues to their normal equilibrium in water we ingest large quantities, and the control of this regula- tion is effected through the sense of thirst. We know little or nothing about the nervous apparatus involved; but it may be assumed that when the water content falls below a certain amount CUTANEOUS AND INTERNAL SENSATIONS. 269 the nerve fibers in the pharyngeal membrane (fibers of the glosso- pharyngeal nerve) are stimulated and give us the sensation of thirst. That we have in this membrane a special end-organ of thirst is indicated, moreover, by the fact that local drying in this region, from dry or salty food, or dry and dusty air, produces a sensation of thirst that may be appeased by moistening the mem- brane with a small amount of water not in itself sufficient to relieve a genuine water need of the body. Our normal thirst sensations might be designated, therefore, as pharyngeal thirst, to indicate the probable origin of the sensory stimuli. Prolonged deprivation of water, however, must affect the water content of all the tissues, and under these conditions sensations are experienced whose quality is not that of simple thirst alone, but of pain or suffering. All ac- counts agree that complete deprivation of water for long periods induces intense discomfort, anguish, and possibly mental troubles, and we may suppose that under these conditions sensory nerves are stimulated in many tissues, and that the metabolism in the ner- vous system in addition is directly affected by the loss of water. It is interesting to note that, while in diseases due to a general in- fection, loss of appetite, anorexia, is a frequent symptom, there is no corresponding loss of the sense of thirst. Even in hydrophobia the patient experiences the sensations of thirst, although unable to drink water. CHAPTER XVI. SENSATIONS OF TASTE AND SMELL. The sense of taste is mediated by nerve fibers distributed to parts of the buccal cavity and particularly to parts of the tongue. The most sensitive regions are the tip, the borders, and the posterior portion of the dorsum of the tongue in the region of the circum- vallate papillae. Taste buds and a sense of taste are described also for the soft palate, the epiglottis, and even for the larynx. The sense is not present uniformly over the entire dorsum of the tongue. On the contrary, it has an irregular, punctiform distribution over most of this region with the exception of the parts mentioned above. The Nerves of Taste. — The anterior two-thirds of the tongue are supplied with sensory fibers from the lingual nerve, a branch of the inferior maxillary division of the fifth nerve, and the posterior third from the glossopharyngeal. The taste fibers for these regions, therefore, are supplied immediately through these nerves. It has been shown, moreover, that the taste fibers carried in the lingual are brought to it through the chorda tympani nerve, which arises from the seventh cranial nerve and joins the lingual soon after emerging from the tympanic cavity of the ear. There has been much discussion as to the origin of these taste fibers from the brain. At first sight it would seem that the fibers for the posterior third of the tongue must have their origin from the brain in the glosso- pharyngeal and those for the anterior two-thirds in the sensory portion of the facial. Many surgeons have reported, however, that complete extirpation of the Gasserian ganglion of the fifth nerve is followed by complete loss of taste in the corresponding side of the tongue, and others have described a loss of taste for the anterior two- thirds following a similar operation. Some authors have asserted, therefore, that all the taste fibers originate or rather end in the sensory nucleus of the fifth, while others believe that the fibers running in the chorda tympani, at least, take their origin in the fifth nerve. It is supposed by these authors that the fibers reach the Gasserian ganglion by a circuitous route, as is indicated in the diagram given in Fig. 113. Those that run in the lingual and chorda tympani nerves are assumed to pass to the ganglion by way of the great superficial petrosal and Vidian nerves and Meckel's ganglion, while those that are contained in the glosso- 270 SENSATIONS OF TASTE AND SMELL. 271 pharyngeal reach the same ganglion through the nerve of Jacobson, the small superficial petrosal, and the otic ganglion. A recent re- port by Cushing,* of the results of removal of the Gasserian gan- glion in thirteen cases, throws much doubt upon these views. This author made careful examinations of the sense of taste, not only immediately after the operation, but for a long period subsequently. He states that in no case was there any effect upon the sense of taste in the posterior third of the tongue. We may believe, therefore, that the taste fibers of this part arise immediately from the ganglion cells in the petrosal ganglion and enter the brain with the roots of the nerve to terminate in its sensory nucleus in the medulla. Regard- ^ptftos.swptffitxoli ^eTrosum.X'i •"S^eus Fig. 113- — Schema to show the course of the taste fibers from tongue to brain. — (.Cushing.) The dotted lines represent the course as indicated by Cushing's observations. The full black lines indicate the paths by which some authors have supposed that these -fibers enter the brain in the trigeminal nerve. ing the anterior two-thirds of the tongue, the lingual region, it was found that in some cases there was at first a loss of acuity of taste or even an entire disappearance of the sense, but subsequently it returned. It would seem, therefore, that the loss of taste de- scribed after removal of the Gasserian ganglion is an incidental result the cause of which is not entirely clear. Cushing attributes it to a postoperative degeneration and swelling in the fibers of the lingual nerve, which affect the conductivity of the intermingled fibers of the chorda tympani. Since, however, there is no perma- * Cushing, " Bulletin of the Johns Hopkins Hospital," 14, 71, 1903. Gives also the surgical literature. 272 THE SPECIAL SENSES. nent loss of taste in this region, it follows that the taste fibers do not pass through the Gasserian ganglion. We may assume, there- fore, that they originate directly in the nerve cells of the geniculate ganglion and enter the brain with the fibers of the portio intermedia of the seventh nerve. The End-organ of the Taste Fibers. — In the circumvallate papillae, in some of the fungiform papillae, and in other portions of the fauces, palate, epiglottis, or even the vocal cords there are found the organs known as taste buds which are believed to act as peripheral organs of taste. These curious structures are repre- sented in Fig. 114. They are oval bodies with an external layer of tegmental or cortical cells, and they contain in the interior a number of elongated cells each of which ends in a hair- like process which projects through the central taste pore of the organ. These latter cells may be consid- ered as the true sense cells ; the hair-like process con- stitutes probably the part that is stimulated directly by sapid substances. The impulse thus aroused is communicated through the body of the cell to the endings of the taste fibers which terminate around these cells by terminal arborizations of the same general type as in the case of the hair cells in the cochlea. Classification of Taste Sensations. — Our taste sensations are very numerous, but it has been shown that there are four primary or fundamental sensations, — namely, sweet, bitter, acid, and salty, and that all other tastes are combinations of these primary sensations, or combinations of one or more of them with sensations of odor or with sensations derived from stimulation of the so-called nerves of common sensibility in the tongue. Thus, the taste of pepper may be resolved into a slight odor sensation and a sensation due to stimulation of the fibers of general sensi- bility,— that is, it gives no taste sensation proper. The taste of alum may be considered as a combination of a salty taste with common sensibility. Combinations of sweet and acid tastes, sweet Fig. 114. — Section through one of the taste buds of the papilla foliata of the rabbit (from Quain, after Ranvier), highly magnified: p, Gus- tatory pore: s, gustatory cell; r, sustentacular cell ; m, leucocyte containing granules ; e, super- ficial epithelial cells; n, nerve fibers. SENSATIONS OF TASTE AND SMELL. 273 and bitter tastes, etc., form a part of our daily experience, and in the fused or compound sensation that results from such com- binations one may usually recognize without difficulty the con- stituent parts. The seemingly great variety of our taste sensations is largely due to the fact that we confuse them or combine them with simultaneous odor sensations. Thus, the flavors in fruits and the bouquet of wines are due to odor sensations which we designate ordinarily as tastes, since they are experienced at the time these objects are ingested. If care is taken to shut off the nasal cavities during the act of ingestion even imperfectly, as by holding the nose, the so-called taste disappears in large measure. Very dis- agreeable tastes are usually, as a matter of fact, due to unpleasant odor sensations. On the other hand, some volatile substances which enter the mouth through the nostrils and stimulate the taste organs are interpreted by us as odors. The odor of chloro- form, for instance, is largely due to stimulation of the sweet taste in the tongue. Distribution and Specific Energy of the Fundamental Taste Sensations. — Regarding the distribution of the funda- mental taste sensations over the tongue and palate there seem to be many individual differences. In general, however, it may be said that the bitter taste is more developed at the back of the tongue and the adjacent or posterior regions; at the tip of the tongue the bitter sense is less marked or in cases may be absent altogether. On the contrary, in this latter region the sweet taste is well developed. On this account it may happen that substances which when first taken into the mouth give a not unpleasant sweet taste subsequently when swallowed cause disagreeably bitter sen- sations, like the little book of the evangelist, which in the mouth was "sweet as honey, and as soon as I had eaten it, my belly was bitter.' ' Oehrwall * has made an interesting series of experiments in which he stimulated separately a number of fungiform papillae on the surface of the tongue. Each papilla was stimulated sepa- rately for its fundamental taste senses of sweet, bitter, and acid, by using drops of solutions of sugar, quinin, and tartaric acid. Of the 125 papillae thus examined, 27 gave no reaction at all, although sensitive to pressure and temperature. In the 98 papillae that reacted to the sapid stimulation it was found that 60 gave taste sensations of all three qualities, 4 gave only sweet and bitter, 7 only bitter and acid, 12 only sweet and acid, 12 only acid, and 3 only sweet. None was found to give only a bitter sensation. These facts bear directly upon the question of the specific energy of the taste fibers. It is possible that the four fundamental taste qualities may be mediated by four different end-organs and four separate * Oehrwall, " Skandinavisches Archiv f. Physiologie," 2, 1, 1890. 18 ?/,. 274 \~^J^ THE SPECIAL SENSES. ers, each giving, when stimulated, only its own quality of sensation. On the other hand, it is possible that one and the same nerve fiber might give different qualities of sensation according to the nature and mode of action of the sapid substances. The fact, as shown by Oehrwall's experiments, that there are sensory spots upon the tongue which will not react to some kinds of sapid substance, but do react to, others, and perhaps only to one particular kind, speaks strongly in favor of the view that there are different end-organs and nerve fibers for each fundamental taste. This view is still further supported by the fact that certain chemically pure sub- stance s give different tastes according to the part of the tongue upon which they are placed. Thus, sodium sulphate (Guyot) may taste salty upon the tip of the tongue and bitter when placed upon the posterior part. A better instance still is given by solutions of a bromin substitution product of saccharin, the chemical name {CO oX ^/ NH. S02/ When this substance is placed upon the tip of the tongue it gives a sweet sensation, while upon the posterior region it gives only a bitter taste together with a sensation of astringency (Howell and Kastle). Extracts of the leaves of a tropical plant, Gymnema silvestre, applied to the tongue, destroy the sense of taste for sweet and bitter sub- stances (Shore), and this fact may be explained most satisfactorily by assuming that this substance exercises a selective action upon separate terminals in the tongue, paralyzing those for the bitter and the sweet substances. Finally, the fact that electrical, me- chanical, or chemical stimulation of the chorda tympani, where it passes through the tympanic cavity, may arouse taste sensations is proof that the taste sensation in general is not due to a peculiar kind of impulse that can be aroused only by the action of sapid bodies upon the terminals in the tongue, but, on the contrary, that it is a specific energy of these fibers, and depends for its quality, there- fore, upon the specific reaction of the terminations in the brain. Method of Sapid Stimulation. — In order that sapid substances may react upon the taste terminals it is necessary, in the first place, that they shall be in solution. It is impossible to taste with a dry tongue. We may assume, therefore, that the stimulation consists essentially in a chemical reaction between the sapid substance and the terminal of the taste fiber, — for instance, the hair process of the sense cells in the taste .buds, — and the question naturally arises whether the distinctive reactions corresponding to the separate taste qualities can be referred to a definite chemical structure in the sapid bodies. Are there certain chemical groups which possess the property of reacting specifically with the end-organs? Experience shows that substances of very different chemical constitution may SENSATIONS OF TASTE AND SMEL^^^y. ^ 275 excite the same taste. Thus, sugar, saccharin, and BUgte^BtjEgfa v ^ (lead acetate) all give a sweet taste, while, on the other hand, starch (soluble starch), which stands so close in structure to the sugars, has no effect upon the taste terminals. It is interesting to remember that the taste nerves may be stimulated by sapid sub- stances dissolved in the blood as well as when applied to the ex- terior of the tongue. A sweet taste may be experienced in diabetes from the sugar in the blood, or a bitter taste in jaundice from the bile. The Threshold Stimulus. — The determination of the threshold stimulus for different sapid substances is made by ascertaining the minimal concentration of the solution which is capable of arousing a taste sensation. The delicacy of the sense of taste is influenced, however, by certain accessory conditions which must be taken into account. Thus, the temperature of the solution is an important condition. Very cold or very hot solutions do not react, — that is, the extremes of temperature seem to diminish or destroy the sensi- tiveness of the end-organ. A temperature between 10° and 30° CL gives the optunum reaction. So also the delicacy of the sense of taste is increased by rubbing the sapid solution against the tongue. Doubtless this mechanical action facilitates the penetration of the sapid body into the mucous membrane, but it seems also to in- crease the irritability of the end-organ. It is our habit in tasting bodies with the tongue to rub this organ against the hard palate. With regard to the threshold stimulus such results as the following are reported: Salty (sodium chlorid). 0.25 gm. in 100 c.c. H20 — detectible on tip of tongue. Sweet (sugar) 0.50 " " " " detectible on tip of tongue. Acid (HC1) 0.007 " " " " detectible on border of tongue. Bitter (quinin) 0.00005 " " " " detectible on root of tongue. The very great sensitiveness of the tongue to bitter substances is evident from this table. The Olfactory Organ. — The end-organ for the olfactory sense lies in the upper part of the nose, and consists of elongated, epithe- lial-like cells, each of which bears on its free end a tuft of six to eight hair-like processes, while at its basal end it is continued into a nerve fiber that passes through the cribriform plate of the ethmoid bone and ends in the olfactory bulb. These olfactory sense cells lie among supporting epithelial cells of a columnar shape (Fig. 115). At the free edge of the cells there is a limiting membrane through which the olfactory hairs project. The olfactory sense 276 THE SPECIAL SENSES. Cells are essentially nerve cells, and in this respect resemble the sense cells in the retina, the rods and cones, rather than those of the ear or of the organs of taste. The distribution of the olfactory cells, according to v. Brunn, is confined to the nasal septum and a portion of the upper turbinate bone. The area covered in each nos- tril corresponds to about 250 square millimeters. The epithelium of the lower and middle turbinates and the floor of the nostrils is composed of the usual ciliated cells found in the respiratory passages, while the so-called vestibular region of the nose, the part roofed in by the cartilage, is covered 'h hWr- by a stratified pavement epithelium corresponding in structure with that of the skin. These latter portions of the nose are supplied with sensory fibers derived from the fifth or trigeminal nerve. We must consider the 500 sq. mm. of olfac- tory epithelium as the olfactory sense organ com- parable physiologically and perhaps anatomically to the rod and cone layer of the retina. The connections of these cells with the central nervous system have al- ready been described (p. 202). It will be remem- bered that the fine, non- medullated fibers springing from the basal end of the sense cells enter the olfac- tory bulb and end in ter- minal arborizations in the olfactory glomeruli, where they make con- nections by contact with the dendrites of the mitral cells of the bulb. Through the axons of these mitral cells the impulses are con- ducted along the olfactory tract to their various terminations in the olfactory lobe itself, either of the same or of the opposite side, and eventually also in the cortical region, the uncinate gyrus, of the hippocampal lobe. As regards the olfactory sense cells, the nerve cells in the olfactory bulb might be compared with the nerve gan- glion layer of the retina, and the nerve fibers of the olfactory tract with the fibers of the optic nerve. The Mechanism of Smelling. — Odoriferous substances to Fig. 115. — Cells of the olfactory region (after v. Brunn) : a, a, Olfactory cells ; 6, b, epithelial cells; n, n, central process prolonged as an olfac- tory nerve fibril; I, I, nucleus; c, knob-like clear termination of peripheral process; h, h, bunch of olfactory hairs. SENSATIONS OF TASTE AND SMELL. 277 affect the olfactory cells must, of course, penetrate into the upper part of the nasal chamber. This end is attained during inspiration, either by simple diffusion or by currents produced by the act of sniffing. It may also happen by way of the posterior nares. In fact, the flavors of many foods, fruits, wine, etc., are olfactory rather than gustatory sensations. When such food is swallowed the poste- rior nares are shut off from the pharynx by the soft palate, but in the expiration succeeding the swallow the odor of the food is con- veyed to the olfactory end-organ. Flavors are perceived, therefore, not during the act of swallowing, but subsequently, and if the nostrils are blocked, as in coryza, foods lose much of their flavor. Simply holding the nose will destroy much of the so-called taste of fruits or the bouquet of wines.* Nature of the Olfactory Stimulus. — The fact that smells are transmitted through space like light and sound has suggested the possibility that they may depend upon a vibratory movement of some medium. This view, although occasionally defended in modern times, is apparently entirely incompatible with the facts. The usual view is that odoriferous bodies emit particles which, as a rule, at least are in gaseous form. These particles are con- veyed to the olfactory epithelium by currents in the air or by simple gaseous diffusion, and after solution in the moisture of the membrane act chemically upon the sensitive hairs of the sense cells. All vapors or gases are, however, not capable of acting as stimuli to these cells; so that evidently the odoriferous character depends upon some peculiarity of structure. It is assumed that there are certain groups, "odoriphore groups," which are character- istic of all odoriferous substances and by virtue of which these substances react with the special form of protoplasm found in the hair cells. Haycraftf has formulated certain fundamental conceptions bearing upon the relation between chemical structure and odoriferous stimulation. He has shown that the power to cause smell, like other physical properties, is a periodic function of the atomic weight — that in the periodic system, according to Men- delejeff, the elements in certain groups are characterized by their odoriferous properties; for instance, the second, fourth, and sixth members — sulphur, selenium, and tellurium — of the sixth group. Moreover, in organic compounds belonging to an homologous series the smell gradually changes and, indeed, increases in the higher members of the series, — that is, in those having a more complex molecular structure. The Qualities of the Olfactory Sensations. — While we dis- * For many interesting; facts concerning smelling and the literature to 1895 see Zwaardemaker, "Die Physiologie des Geruchs," Leipzig, 1895. fHaycraft, "Brain," 1888, p. 166. 278 the special senses. tinguish a great many different kinds of odors, it has been found difficult, indeed impossible, to classify them very satisfactorily into groups. That is, it is not possible to pick out what might be called the fundamental odor sensations. This sense was doubtless used by primitive man chiefly in detecting and testing food, in protect- ing himself from noxious surroundings, and perhaps also in controll- ing his social relations. The olfactory sensations, in accordance with this use made of them, give either pleasant or unpleasant sensa- tions in a more marked and universal way than in the case of vision or hearing, approaching, in this respect, rather the purely sensual characteristics of the lower senses, the bodily appetites. Mankind has been content to classify odors as agreeable and disagreeable, and to designate the many different qualities of odors by the names of the substances which in his individual experience usually give rise to them. A number of observers have proposed classifications more or less complete in character. One of the latest and perhaps the best is that suggested by Zwaardemaker on the basis of the nomenclatures introduced by previous observers. Adopting first the general grouping into pure odors, odors mixed with sensa- tions of common sensibility from the mucous membrane of the nose, and odors mixed or confused with tastes, he separates the pure odors or odors proper into nine classes, as follows : I. Odores setherei or ethereal odors, such as are given by the fruits, and depend upon the presence of ethereal substances or esters. II. Odores aromatici or aromatic odors, which are typified by camphor and citron, bitter almond and the resinous bodies. This class is divided into five subgroups. III. Odores fragrantes, the fragrant or balsamic odors, comprising the vari- ous flower odors or perfumes. The class falls into three subgroups. IV. Odores ambrosiaci, the ambrosial odors, typified by amber and musk. This odor is present in the flesh, blood, or excrement of some ani- mals, being referable in the last instance to the bile. V. Odores alliacei or garlic odors, such as are found in the onion, garlic, sulphur, selenium and tellurium compounds. They fall into three subgroups. VI. Odores empyreumatici or the burning odors, the odors given by roasted coffee, baked bread, tobacco smoke, etc. The odors of benzol, phenol, and the products of dry distillation of wood come into this class. VII. Odores hircini or goat odors. The odor of this animal arises from* the caproic and caprylic acid contained in the sweat; cheese, sweat, spermatic and vaginal secretions give odors of a similar quality. VIII. Odores tetri or repulsive odors, such as are given by many of the nar- cotic plants and acanthus. IX. Odores nauseosi or nauseating or fetid odors, such as are given by feces and certain plants and the products of putrefaction. While the classification serves to emphasize a number of marked resemblances or relations that exist among the odors, it does not rest wholly upon a subjective kinship, — that is, the different odors brought together in one class do not in all cases arouse in us sensa- SENSATIONS OF TASTE AND SMELL. 279 tions that seem to be of related quality. It is not impossible, how- ever, that further analysis may succeed in showing that there are certain fundamental qualities in our numerous odor sensations. Our position regarding the odors is similar to that which formerly prevailed in the case of the taste sensations. It was thought to be impossible to classify these latter satisfactorily on the basis of a few fundamental sensations, but it is now universally accepted that all of our true gustatory sensations show one or more of four primary taste qualities. As was said above, our odor sensations are classi- fied in ordinary life as agreeable or disagreeable, and, indeed, Haller, the great physiologist of the eighteenth century, divided odors along this line into three classes: (1) the agreeable or am- brosial, (2) the disagreeable or fetid, and (3) the mixed odors. In many cases, no doubt, the agreeableness or disagreeableness of an odor depends solely upon the associations connected with it. If the associative memories aroused are unpleasant the odor is dis- agreeable. Thus, the odor of musk, so pleasant to most persons, produces most disagreeable sensations in others, on account of past associations. It is possible, however, that there is some funda- mental difference in physiological reaction between such odors as those of putrefaction and of a violet which may be considered as the cause of the difference in psychical effect. It has been suggested, for instance, that they may affect the circulation in the brain in opposite ways, one producing an increased, the other a decreased flow. This improbable supposition has been shown to be devoid of foun- dation by the observations of Shields.* In his experiments the vascu- lar supply to the skin of the arm was determined by plethysmo- graphic methods, and it was found that both pleasant (heliotrope perfume) and unpleasant (putrefactive) odors give a similar vascu- lar reaction. Each class, if it acts at all, causes, as a rule, a con- striction of the skin vessels, such as is obtained normally from in- creased mental activity, — a reaction usually interpreted to mean a greater flow of blood to the brain. Fatigue of the Olfactory Apparatus. — It is a matter of common observation that many odors, such as the perfumes of flowers, quickly cease to give a noticeable sensation when the stimu- lation is continued. This result is usually attributed to fatigue of the sense cells in the end-organ and it is noticeable chiefly with faint odors. One who sits in an ill- ventilated room occupied by many persons may be quite unconscious of the unpleasant odor from the vitiated air, while to a newcomer it is most distinct. It is said that certain classes of odors do not exhibit this property, — the aromatic odors, for example, such as are caused by the cam- phor group. * Shields, " Journal of Experimental Medicine," 1, 1896. 280 THE SPECIAL SENSES. Threshold Stimulus — Delicacy of the Olfactory Sense. — The extraordinary delicacy of the sense of smell in some of the lower animals is seemingly beyond the power of objective measurement or expression. The ability of a dog, for instance, to follow the trail of a given person depends undoubtedly upon the recognition of the individual odor, and the actual amount of olfactory material left upon the ground which serves as the stimulus must be infinitesi- mally small. Even in ourselves the actual amount of olfactory material which suffices to give a distinct sensation is often beyond our means of determination except by the aid of calculation. It is recognized in chemical work, for instance, that traces of known substances too small to give the ordinary chemical reactions may be detected easily by the sense of smell. By taking known amounts Fig. 116. — Zwaardemaker's olfactometer. of odoriferous substances and diluting them to known extents it is possible to express in weights the minimal amount of each substance that can cause a sensation. By this method such figures as the following are obtained : Camphor is perceived in a dilution of 1 part to 400,000; musk, 1 part to 8,000,000; vanillin, 1 part to 10,000,000; while, according to the experiments of Fischer and Penzoldt, mercaptan may be detected in a dilution of ^ -^fa -^-^ of a milli- gram in 1 liter of air or tbto" W7ro"o" °^ a milligram in 50 c.c. of air. Various methods have been proposed to determine the relative delicacy of the olfactory sense in different persons, and these methods have some application in the clinical diagnosis of certain cases. Zwaardemaker has devised a simple apparatus, the olfactometer, the principle of which is illustrated in Fig. 116. It consists of an outside cylinder — the olfactory cylinder, whose inner surface is of SENSATIONS OF TASTE AND SMELL. 281 porous material which can be filled with a known strength of olfac- tory solution — and an inside tube, smelling tube. This latter is applied to the nose and where it runs inside the cylinder it is gradu- ated in centimeters. It is evident that the further out the inner tube is pulled the greater will be the amount of olfactory substance which will be exposed to the incoming air of an inspiration. Conflict of Olfactory Sensations. — When different odors are inhaled simultaneously through the two nostrils they may give rise to the phenomenon of a conflict of the olfactory fields similar to that described for the visual fields. That is, we perceive first one then the other without obtaining a fused or compound sensation. The result depends largely on the odors selected. In some cases one odor may predominate in consciousness to the entire suppression of the other, — a phenomenon which also has an analogy in binocular sensations. It is well known, also, that certain odors antagonize or neutralize others. It is said, for instance, that the odor of iodoform, usually so persistent and so disagreeable, may be neutralized by the addition of Peru balsam, and that the odor of carbolic acid may destroy that of putrefactive processes. Whether the neutralization is of a chemical nature or is physiological does not seem to have been definitely ascertained. Olfactory Associations. — Personal experience shows clearly that olfactory sensations arouse numerous associations — our olfactory memories are good. On the anatomical side the cortical center in the hippocampal lobe is known to be widely connected with other parts of the cerebrum, and we have in this fact a basis for the extensive associations connected with odors. In animals like the dog, with highly developed olfactory organs, it is evident that this sense must play a correspondingly large part in the psychical life. In such animals as well as among the invertebrates it is in- timately connected with the sexual reflexes, and some remnant of this relationship is obvious among human beings. Among the so- called special senses that of smell is perhaps the one most closely connected with the bodily appetites, and overgratifi cation or over- indulgence of this sense, according to historical evidence, has at least been associated with periods of marked decadence of virtue among civilized nations. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE EYE. The eye is the peripheral organ of vision. By means of its peculiar physical structure rays of light from external objects are focused upon the retina and there set up nerve impulses that are transmitted by the fibers of the optic nerve and optic tract to the visual center in the cortex of the brain, where they arouse that reaction in consciousness which we designate as a visual sensation. In studying the physiology of vision we may consider the eye first as an optical instrument physically adapted to form an image on the retina and provided with certain physiological mechanisms for its regulation ; and secondly we may study the properties of the retina in relation to its reactions to light and the visual sensations them- selves, or the physiology of the visual center in the brain. CHAPTER XVII. THE EYE AS AN OPTICAL INSTRUMENT-DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. Formation of an Image by a Biconvex Lens. — That the re- fractive surfaces of the eye form an image of external objects upon its retinal surface is a necessary conclusion from its physical struc- ture. The fact may be demonstrated directly, however, by ob- servation upon the excised eye of an albino rabbit. The thin coats of such an eye are semitransparent, and if the eye is placed in a tube of blackened paper and held in front of one's own eyes it can be seen readily that a small, inverted image of external objects is formed upon the retinal surface, just as an inverted image of the exterior is formed upon the ground glass plate of a photographic camera. This image is formed in the eye by virtue of the refractive surfaces of the cornea and the lens. The curved surfaces of these transparent bodies act substantially like a convex glass lens, and the physics of the formation of an image by such a lens gives the simplest explanation of the refractive processes in the eye. To understand the formation of an image by a biconvex lens the following physical facts must be 282 DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 283 borne in mind. Parallel rays of light falling upon one surface of the lens are brought to a point or focus (F) behind the other surface (Fig. 117). This focus for parallel rays is the principal focus and the distance of this point from the lens is the principal focal distance. This distance depends upon the curvature of the lens and its refractive power, as measured by the refractive index of the material of which it is composed. Parallel rays are given theoretically by a source of light at an infinite distance in front of the lens. Practi- cally any luminous object not nearer than twenty feet gives parallel rays. On the other hand, if a luminous object is placed at F the rays Fig. 117. — Diagrams to illustrate the refraction of light by r. convex lens: a., Refrac- tion of parallel rays; b., refraction of divergent rays; c, refraction of divergent rays from a luminous point nearer than the principal focal distance. from it that strike upon the lens will emerge from the other surface as parallel rays of light. If a luminous point (/, Fig. 117) is placed in front of such a lens at a distance greater than the principal focal distance, but nearer than about twenty feet, the cone of diverging rays from it that impinges upon the surface of the lens will be brought to a focus (/') further away than the principal focus. Conversely the rays from a luminous point at /' will be brought to a focus at /. These points, / and /', are therefore spoken of as conjugate foci. All luminous points within the limits specified will have their cor- responding conjugate foci, at which their images will be formed by 2S4 THE SPECIAL SENSES. the lens. Lastly, if a luminous point is placed at v, Fig. 117. nearer to the lens than the principal focal distance, the cone of strongly divergent rays that falls upon the lens, although refracted, is still divergent after leaving the lens on the other side and consequently is not focused and forms no real image of the point. For every lens there is a point known as the optical center, and for biconvex lenses this point lies within the lens, o. The line joining this center and the principal focus is the principal axis of the lens (o-F, Fig. 117). All other straight lines passing through the optical center are known as secondary a.n s. Rays of light that are coincident with any of these secondary axes suffer no angular deviation in passing through the lens; they emerge parallel to their line of entrance and practically unchanged in direction. Moreover, any luminous point not on the Fig. 118. — Diagrams to illustrate the formation of an image by a biconvex lens: a, For- mation of the image of a point ; b, formation of the images of a- series of points. principal axis will have its image (conjugate focus) formed some- where upon the secondary axis drawn from this point through the optical center. The exact position of the image of such a point can be determined by the following construction (Fig. 118) : Let A represent the luminous point in question. It will throw a cone of rays upon the lens, the limiting rays of which may be represented by A -h and A-c. One of these rays will be parallel, A-p, and will therefore pass through the principal focus, F. If this distance is determined and is indicated properly in the construction, the line A-p may be drawn, as indicated, so as to pass through F after leaving the lens. The point at which the prolongation of this line cuts the secondary axis. A-o, marks the conjugate focus of A and gives the position at which all of the rays will be focused to form the image, a. In DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 285 calculating the position of the image of any object in front of the lens the same method may be followed, the construction being drawn to determine the images for two or more limiting points, as shown in Fig. 118. Let A-B be an arrow in front of the lens. The image of A will be formed at a on the secondary axis A-o, and the image of B at b along the secondary axis B-o. The images of the intervening points will, of course, lie between a and b; so that the image of the entire object will be that of an inverted arrow. This image may be caught on a screen at the distance indicated by the construction if the latter is drawn to scale. The principal focus of a convex lens may be determined experimentally or it may be calculated from the formula -- + \ = j, in which / represents the principal focal dis- tance and p and p1, the conjugate foci for an object farther away than the principal focal distance. That is, if the distance of the object from the lens, p, is known, and the distance of its image, p1, is determined experimentally, the principal focal distance of the lens, /, may be determined by the formula, or if any two of the fac- tors, p, p1, and /, are known the third may be reckoned from the formula. Formation of an Image by the Eye. — As stated above, the re- fractive surfaces of the eye act essentially like a convex lens. As a matter of fact, these refractive surfaces are more complex than in the case of the biconvex lens. In the latter the rays of light suffer refraction at two points only. Where they enter the lens they pass from a rarer to a denser medium and where they leave the lens they pass from a denser to a rarer medium. At these two points, therefore, they are refracted. In the eye there is a larger series of refractive surfaces. The light is refracted at the anterior surface of the cornea, where it passes from the air into the denser medium of the cornea ; at the anterior surface of the lens, where it again enters a denser medium; and at the posterior surface of the lens, where it enters the less dense vitreous humor. The relative refractive powers of these different media have been determined and are expressed in terms of their refractive indices, that of air being taken as unity.* The three points at which the light is refracted are indicated * The term index of refraction expresses the constant ratio between the angles of incidence and of refraction, or specifically between the sine of the angle of incidence and the sine of the angle of refraction: -: = index of sine r refraction. Index of refraction for air = 1 Index of refraction for cornea and aqueous hu- mor = 1.3365 Index of refraction for crystalline lens = 1.4371 Index of refraction for vitreous humor = 1.3365 286 THE SPECIAL SENSES. in the accompanying schema (Fig. 119). The refractive surfaces of the eye may be considered as being composed of a concavo-convex lens, the cornea and aqueous humor, and a biconvex lens, the crystalline lens. In a system of this kind, composed of several refractive media, it has been shown that to construct geometrically the path of the rays it is necessary to know six points ; these are the six car- dinal points or optical con- stants of Gauss, — namely, the anterior and the poste- rior focal distance, the two nodal points, and the two principal points. So far Fig. 119.— Diagram to illustrate the surfaces as the eye is Concerned, it in the eye at which the rays of light are chiefly . , , , , , , . refracted. has been shown that the path of the rays of light may be represented with sufficient accuracy by employing what is known as the reduced schematic eye of Listing, in which the refraction is supposed to take place at a single convex surface separating two media, the air on one side and the media of the eye on the other, the latter having a refractive index of 1.33 (see Fig. 120). In this reduced eye the position of the ideal refracting surface lies in the aqueous humor, at a distance of 2.1 mms. from the anterior surface of the cornea, and the position of the nodal point or optical center — that is, the center of curvature of the ideal refracting surface, c', lies in the crystalline lens at n, a distance of 7.3 mms. from the anterior surface of the . Fig. 120. — Diagram to illustrate the reduced COmea. The principal focal or schematic eye with a single refracting surface .. . \ . \. L. separating two media of different densities: c', distance IOr thlS retracting the ideal refracting surface situated 2.1 mms. e ,. ... . f. behind the anterior surface of real cornea: n, Surface lies at a distance 01 the nodal point, or center of curvature of the OO Q mmc l™l^r^ +V.« on+^ surface c', and 15.5 mms. in front of retina. ll.?S mms. Denma tUe ante- The eyeball is supposed to be filled with a uni- r\nv ciirfapp nf tViA onmvn rtr form substance having a refractive index of 1.33, noi sunace or tne cornea or equal to that of the Yitieoua humor. (22.8 — 7.3) at a distance of 15.5 mms. behind the nodal point. In the eye at rest this principal focal distance coincides with the retina, since the refracting surfaces in the normal resting eye are so formed that parallel rays (rays from distant objects) are brought to a focus on the retina. To show the formation of the image of an external object on the retina it suffices, therefore, to use a construction such as is represented in DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 287 Fig. 121. Secondary axes are drawn from the limiting points of the object — A and B — through the nodal point. Where these axes cut the retina the retinal image of the object will be formed. That is, all the rays of light proceeding from A that penetrate the eye will be focused at a, and all proceeding from B at b. The image on the retina will therefore be inverted and will be smaller than the object. The angle formed at the nodal point by the lines A-n and B-n is known as the visual angle; it varies inversely with the dis- tance of the object from the eye. The Inversion of the Image on the Retina. — Although the images of external objects on the retina are inverted, we see them erect. This fact is easily understood when we remember that our actual visual sensations take place in the brain and that the pro- jection of these sensations to the exterior is a secondary act that has been learned from experience. Experience has taught us to project the visual sensation arising from the stimulus of any given point on the retina to that part of the external world from which the stimulus arises, — that is, to the lumin- ous point giving origin to the light rays. Accord- ing to the physical prin- ciples described above, the image of such a point mUSt be formed On the Fig. m— Diagram to illustrate the construc- t ,, , tion necessary to determine the location and size of retina where the second- the retinal image, ary axis from that point through the nodal point touches the retina. In projecting this retinal stimulus outward to its source, therefore, we have learned to project it back, as it were, along the line of its secondary axis. In Fig. 121 the retinal stimulus at a is projected outward along the line a-n- A, and to such a distance as, from other sources, we estimate the object A to be. This law of projection is fixed by experience, but it implies, as will be noted, that we are conscious of the differences in sensation aroused by stimulation of different parts of the retina. Considering the retina as a sensory surface, — like the skin, for instance, — each point, speaking in general terms, may be assumed to be connected with a definite portion of the cortex, and the sensation aroused by the stimulation of these dif- ferent points must differ to some extent in consciousness, each has its local sign. The sensations arising from each of these points we have learned to project outward into the external world along the line from it to the nodal point of the eye, because under the normal conditions of life this point is stimulated only by external objects 288 THE SPECIAL SENSES. situated on this line. This law of projection is so firmly fixed that if a' given point in the retina is stimulated in some unusual way we still project the resulting sensation outward according to the law, and thus make a false projection and interpretation. For instance, if the little finger is inserted into the inner and lower angle of the eye and is pressed upon the eyeball the edge of the retina is stimulated mechanically. One experiences, in consequence, a visual sensation, known as a phosphene, consisting of a dark-blue spot surrounded by a light halo. This sensation, however, is projected out toward the upper and outer angle of the eye, accord- ing to the law of projection, since normally this part of the retina is only stimulated by light coming from such a direction. A similar error in projection is obtained by holding objects so close to the eye that a physical inverted image cannot be formed, but only an erect shadow image. This experiment may be performed as follows: Hold the head of a pin close to the eye, and, in order that a sharp shadow may be thrown, allow the light to fall on this pin through a pinhole in a card held somewhat farther from the eye. By this means an erect shadow of the pin, lying in the circle of light from the hole, will be thrown on the eye. This shadow image will be projected outward according to the usual law, and consequently will appear inverted. The Size of the Retinal Image. — The size of the image of an object on the retina may be reckoned easily, provided the size of the object and its distance from the eye is known. As will be seen from the construction given in Fig. 121, the triangles A-n-B and a-n-b are symmetrical; consequently we have the ratio: A-B : a-b : : A-n : a-n or A-B A-n +1 . . — — = : that is a-b a-n Size of object Distance of object from nodal point. Size of image " " Distance of image from nodal point. As was stated above, the distance of the image from the nodal point — that is, the distance of the retina from the nodal point — may be placed at 15.5 or 15 mms. Consequently, three of the factors in the above equation being known, it is easily solved for the un- known factor — namely, the size of the image on the retina. To take a concrete example; suppose it is desired to know the size on the retina of the image made by an object 120 feet high at a distance of one mile (5280 feet) . If we designate the size of the image as x and substitute the known values for the other terms of the equation, we have = -=■=-. or x = 0.341 mm., which is about the diame- x 15 7 ; ter of the fovea centralis. The retinal image of the object in DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 289 this case would be, in round numbers, about nnrVini- °f "the actual size of the object. Accommodation of the Eye for Objects at Different Dis- tances.— The normal or, as it is sometimes named, the emmetropic eye, is arranged to focus parallel rays more or less accurately upon the retina. That is, the refractive media have such curvatures and densities that parallel, or substantially parallel rays are brought to a focus upon the retinal surface. When objects are brought closer to the eye, however, the rays proceeding from them become more and more divergent. If the eye remains unchanged the refracted rays cut the retina before coming to a focus — so that each luminous point in the object, instead of forming a point upon the Fig. 122. — Diagram explaining the change in the position of the image reflected from the anterior surface of the crystalline lens. — (Williams, after Donders.) retina, forms a circle, known as a diffusion circle. As this is true for each point of the object, the retinal image as a whole is blurred. We know, however, that up to a certain point at least this blurring does not occur when the object is brought closer to the eyes. The eye, in fact, accommodates itself to the nearer object so as to obtain a clear focus. In a photographic camera this accommodation or focusing is effected by moving the ground glass plate farther away as the object is brought closer to the lens. In the eye the same result is obtained by increasing the curva- ture and therefore the refractive power, of the lens. That a change in the lens is the essential factor in accommodation for near objects is demonstrated by a simple and conclusive experiment devised by 19 290 THE SPECIAL SENSES. Helmholtz with the aid of what are known as the images of Pur- kinje. The principle of this experiment is represented by the dia- gram given in Fig. 122. The eye to be observed is relaxed; that is, gazes into the distance. A lighted candle is held to one side as represented, and the observer places his eye so as to catch the light of the candle when reflected from the observed eye. With a little practice and under the right conditions of illumina- tion the observer will be able to see three images of the candle re- flected from the observed eye : one, the brightest, is reflected from the convex surface of the cornea (a, Fig. 123, A) ; one much dimmer and of larger size is reflected from the convex surface of the lens (6, Fig. 123, A). This image is larger and fainter because the re- flecting surface is less curved. The third image (c, Fig. 123, A) is inverted and is smaller and brighter than the second. This image is reflected from the posterior surface of the lens, which acts, in this instance, like a concave mirror. If now the observed eye A B Fig. 123. — Reflected images of a candle flame as seen in the pupil of an eye at rest and accommodated for near objects. — {Williams.) gazes at a near object it will be noted (Fig. 123, B) that the first image does not change at all, the third image also remains practi- cally the same, but the middle image (6) becomes smaller and ap- proaches nearer to the first (a). This result can only mean that in the act of accommodation the anterior surface of the lens becomes more convex. In this way its refractive power is increased and the more divergent rays from the near object are focused on the retina. Helmholtz has shown that the curvature of the posterior surface of the lens is also increased slightly ; but the change is so slight that the increased refractive power is referred chiefly to the change in the anterior surface. The means by 'which the change is effected was first explained satisfactorily by Helmholtz.* He attributed it to the contraction of the ciliary muscle. This small muscle, composed of plain muscle fibers, is found within the eyeball, lying * Helmholtz, "Handbuch der phvsiologischen Optik," second edition, 1896. DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 291 between the choroid and the sclerotic coat at the point at which the sclerotic passes into the cornea and the choroid falls into the ciliary processes. Some of its fibers take a more or less circular direction around the eyeball, resembling thus a sphincter muscle, while others take a radial direction in the plane of the meridians of the eye and have their insertion in the choroid coat (Fig. 124). When this muscle contracts the radial fibers especially will pull forward the choroid coat. The effect of this change in the choroid is to loosen the pull of the suspensory ligament (zonula Zinnii) on the lens and this organ then bulges forward by its own elasticity. The theory assumes that in a condition of- rest the suspensory ligament, which runs from the ciliary processes to the capsule of the lens, exerts a Pigment-free Ciliary Border epithelium. process, of iris. Ciliary muscle. Lens. Pigment ,.' epithelium. Ora serrata. Fig. 124. — Meridional section of eyeball after removal of sclerotic coat, cornea, and iris, to show the position of the ciliary muscle. — (Schultze.) tension upon the lens which keeps it flattened, particularly along its anterior surface, since the ligament is attached more to this side. When this tension is relieved indirectly by the contraction of the ciliary muscle the elasticity of the lens, or rather of the capsule of the lens, causes it to assume a more spherical shape along its anterior surface, and the amount of this change is proportional to the extent of contraction of the muscle. Other theories have been proposed to explain the way in which the contraction of the ciliary muscle effects a change in the curvature of the lens,* but none is so simple and, on the whole, so satisfactory as the one suggested by Helmholtz. * See Tscherning, "Optique physiologique," Paris, 1898; and Schoen, "Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie," 59, 427, 1895. 292 THE SPECIAL SENSES. Limit of the Power of Accommodation — Near Point of Distinct Vision. — When an object is brought closer and closer to the eye a point will be reached at which it is impossible by the strongest contraction of the ciliary muscle to obtain a clear image of the object. The rays from it are so divergent that the refractive surfaces are unable to bring them to a focus on the retina. Each luminous point makes a diffusion circle on the retina, and the whole image is indistinct. The distance at which the eye is just able to accommodate and within which distinct vision is impos- sible is called the near point Observation shows that this near point varies steadily with age and becomes rapidly greater in dis- tance between the fortieth and the fiftieth year. In the case of the normal eye the recession of the near point varies so regularly with age that it is said that its determination may be used to estimate the age of the individual. Figures of this kind are given: Age. 10 20 30 40 50 60 Near Point. 7 cm. or 2.76 in 10 " " 3.94 " 14 " " 5.61 " 22 " " 8.66 " 40 " " 15.75 " 100 " " 39.37 " This gradual lengthening of the near point is explained usually by the supposition that the lens loses its elasticity, so that con- traction of the ciliary muscle has less and less effect in causing an increase in its curvature. The process starts very early in life, and is one of the many facts which show that senescence begins practically with birth. The change in near point in early life is so slight as to escape notice, but after it reaches a distance of about 25 cm. (about 10 inches) the fact obtrudes itself upon us in the use of our eyes for near objects, — reading, for example. The condition is then designated as old-sightedness or presbyopia. Most normal eyes become so distinctly presbyopic between the fortieth and the fiftieth year as to require the use of glasses in reading. If no other defect exists in the eye, this deficiency of the lens is readily over- come by using suitable convex glasses to aid the eye in focusing the rays. It is obvious that in such cases the glasses need not be used except for near work. Far Point of Distinct Vision. — The normal eye is so adjusted that parallel rays are brought to a focus on the retina. The far point is therefore theoretically at infinity. Objects at a great distance are seen distinctly, as far as their size permits, without accommodation, — that is, with the eye at rest. Practically it is found that objects at a distance of 6 to 10 meters (20 to 30 feet) send rays that are sufficiently parallel to focus on the retina without DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 293 muscular effort on the part of the eyes, and this distance, therefore, measures the practical far point, punctum remotum, of the normal eye. The rays at this distance are, in reality, somewhat divergent, and that they produce a distinct image without an act of accom- modation may be due to the fact that the rods and cones, the really sensitive part of the retina, do not form a mathematical plane, but have a certain thickness or depth. In the fovea centralis, for in- stance, the cones have a length estimated (Greeff) at 85 fi (0.085 mm.), and since the displacement of the focus of an object moved from an infinite distance (parallel rays) to 6 or 10 meters from the eye is less than this amount, the focused image would continue to fall on some part of the cones without the aid of the mechanism of accommodation. The Refractive Power of the Surfaces in the Eye. — The refractive power of lenses is expressed usually in terms of their principal focal distance, a lens with a distance of one meter being taken as the unit and designated as having a refractive power of one diopter, 1 D. Compared with this unit, the refractive power of lenses is expressed in terms of the reciprocal of their principal focal dis- tance measured in meters; thus; a lens with a principal focal dis- tance of yV meter is a lens of 10 diopters, 10 D., and one with a focal distance of 10 meters is TV diopter (0.1 D.). The posterior prin- cipal focal distance of the combination of refractive surfaces in the eye is 22.7 mms. or ||^ meters. The reciprocal of this length of focus, ~y or 44.05 D., expresses the refractive power of the eye under the normal conditions in which the rays are refracted into the dense vitreous humor. As compared with a lens in air, the refractive power of the eye would be expressed by multiplying this figure by the index of refraction of the vitreous humor, 44.05 X 1.3665 = 58.8 D., the figure usually given to express the total refractive power. The refractive power of the crystalline lens alone is 16 D.; that of the cornea, 43 D. ; hence the latter surface has about two and one- half times the refractive power of the lens. Removal of the lens, therefore, as in cataract operations, does not lessen the refractive power of the eye so much as when the action of the cornea is de- stroyed, as happens in part when the head is immersed in water. Optical Defects of the Normal Eye.— The refractive surfaces of the eye exhibit some of the optical defects commonly noticed in lenses, particularly those defects known as chromatic and spherical aberration. White light is composed of ether waves of different lengths and different rapidities of vibration, the shortest waves being those at the violet end of the spectrum and the longest those at the red end. In passing through a prism or lens these waves are re- fracted unequally and are therefore more or less dispersed accord- 294 THE SPECIAL SENSES. ing to the character of the refracting medium. The short, rapid waves at the violet end are refracted the most and are brought to a focus before the longer, red waves, so that the image shows fringes of color instead of being pure white. This phenomenon is known as chromatic aberration. Lenses used for scientific purposes are corrected for this defect or made achromatic by a combination of lenses of crown and flint glass so placed that the dispersive power of one neutralizes that of the other. The eye exhibits this defect, but not to such an extent as to be noticeable in ordinary vision. If, however, an object is in focus when viewed by red light it can be shown that the focus must be changed if the same object is illuminated by violet light. Helmholtz estimates that if the media of the eye possess the same dispersive power as water the rays of violet light must be brought to a focus at about 0.434 mm. in front of that of the red rays. Spherical aberration depends upon the fact that the rays near the circumference of a lens are refracted more and therefore are brought to a focus sooner than those entering nearest the center. This defect may be noticed in an uncorrected lens by the fact that when the center of the image is in exact focus its margins are slightly out of focus and vice versa. The defect is usually cor- rected, as in photography, by use of a diaphragm to cut off the rays from the periphery of the lens. In the eye both spher- ical and chromatic aberrations are remedied to a large extent by a similar device. The iris constitutes an adjustable diaphragm, which is reflexly narrowed as the light increases in intensity and thus cuts off the rays that would go through the periphery of the lens. The interesting physiological control of the movements of the iris is described below. In the eye the defect of spherical aberration is counteracted also by the peculiar structure of the crystalline lens. This organ is composed of concentric layers whose density increases toward the center. The result of this arrangement is that the center of the lens is more refractive than the periphery, and the tendency of the latter portion to refract more strongly is more or less neutralized. A third optical defect of the eye consists in the fact that its refractive surfaces are not absolutely centered, — that is, the centers of curvature of the cornea and of the anterior and the posterior surfaces of the lens do not lie in the same straight line. Moreover, the optical axis of the system does not coincide exactly with the line of sight. By the latter term we mean the line from the point looked at to the fovea centralis or the part of the fovea on which the image of the point falls. This line of sight or visual axis makes an angle of about five degrees with the optical axis. The system would be more perfect as an optical apparatus if the two axes coincided. DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 295 Abnormalities in the Refraction of the Eye — Ametropia. — The eye that is normal and in which parallel rays focus on the retina when the eye is at rest is designated as emmetropic. Any abnormality in the refractive surfaces or the shape of the eyeball prevents this exact focusing of parallel rays and makes the eye ametropic. The most common troubles of the eye are due to short-sightedness or myopia, far-sightedness or hyperme- tropia, astigmatism, old-sightedness or presbyopia, and lack of balance in the external muscles of the eyeballs, muscular inefficiency or heterophoria. Some description of these conditions is useful to emphasize by contrast the mode of action of the dioptric mechanism in the normal eye, but for a full description of the extent and com- plexity of these defects reference must be made to special treatises upon the errors of refraction in the eye. In myopia or near-sightedness parallel rays of light are brought to a focus before reaching the retina. Consequently when the rays fall upon the retina each point forms a diffusion circle and the image is indistinct. This defect may be due to an abnormally great curvature of the refractive surfaces, the cornea or the lens, or to an abnormal length of the eyeball in its anteroposterior diameter. The last cause is the most common. The defect may be congenital, but usually it is acquired, and in the latter case its cause is generally attributed to a weakness in the coats of the eyeball. The interior of the eye is under some pressure, intra-ocular tension, which is estimated to be equal to the pressure of a column of mercury 25 to 30 mms. in height. This tension is increased by strong convergence of the eyeballs in looking at near objects. If the coats of the eye are weak or become so from disease or malnutrition they may yield somewhat to this pressure and the eyeball become lengthened in the anteroposterior diameter. The condition as regards refraction of parallel rays is represented then by the diagram B, in Fig. 125. The retina is farther away than the principal focal distance of the refractive surfaces, and if the defect is excessive even diverging rays may not be focused. The obvious remedy for such a condition is to use concave lenses before the eyes for distant vision. By this means, if the lenses are properly chosen, the rays will be given such an amount of divergence that the focus will be thrown back to the retina. As compared with the normal or emmetropic eye, the myopic eye has its far point of distinct vision — that is, the farthest point that can be seen distinctly without an effort of accommo- tion — less than twenty feet from the eye, the exact distance depend- ing upon the extent of the myopia. On the contrary, the near point of distinct vision — that is, the nearest point at which distinct vision can be obtained with the aid of the muscles of accommodation — is closer than in the normal eye. Much of the prevalent myopia in the 296 THE SPECIAL SENSES. young is attributed by oculists to bad methods in reading, such as insufficient lighting, small print, and a faulty position of the book. Such conditions lead to an excessive muscular effort and thus aggravate any tendency that exists toward the development of a near-sighted condition. In hypermetropia the conditions are the opposite of those in myopia. Parallel rays of light after refraction in the eye cut the retina before they come to a focus. The principal focal distance, in other words, is behind the retina. In this case, also, each point of a distant object will make upon the retina, when the eye is not accommodated, a dif- fusion circle, and the image consequently is blurred. This defect may be caused by a lessened curvature or refractive power in the cornea or lens, but in the majority of cases it is ref- erable to a diminution in the anteroposterior diam- eter of the eyeball. This condition is usually con- genital: the eyeball from birth is smaller than the normal. The path of the parallel rays in this case is represented in the dia- gram C, Fig. 125. When such an eye looks at a dis- tant object a clear image may be obtained only by using the ciliary muscle, and to prevent this constant strain upon the muscle of accommo- dation convex glasses must be worn. Glasses of this kind converge the rays and if properly chosen will bring parallel rays to a focus without the constant aid of accommodation. It is obvious that in the hypermetropic eye there is no far point of distinct vision when the eye is at rest, since some accommodation must be used to bring even parallel rays to a focus. The near point of distinct vision will be farther away than in the normal eye, since accommodation be- gins when the rays are parallel and its limits are reached with a less degree of divergence; hence the name of far-sightedness. Fig. 125. — Diagram showing the difference be- tween normal (;1), myopic (B), and hypermetropic (C) eyes. In B and C the dotted lines represent the path of the rays after correction by glasses. — (Bow- ditch.) DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 297 Presbyopia or old-sightedness has been referred to above. It is due to a gradual failure in the effectiveness of accommodation with increasing age, and is attributed usually to a progressive loss of elasticity in the lens. The near point of distinct vision recedes farther and farther from the eye, and consequently in close work convex glasses must be worn to aid the accommodation. It is obvious that this effect of old age will be less noticeable in the myopic than in the emmetropic eye, since in the former the greater length of the eyeball needs less accommodation in near vision and the failure of the lens to refract is therefore not felt so soon. What is known as second-sight in the old may be brought about by the late development of a myopic condition, — that is, by a change in the length of the eyeball or by a swelling of the crystalline lens, — and in such a case convex glasses for near work may be dispensed with. Astigmatism. — In a perfectly normal or ideal eye the refractive surfaces, cornea, anterior and posterior surfaces of the lens, are sections of true spheres, and, all the meridians being of equal curvature, the refraction along these different meridians is equal. Such an eye will bring the cone of rays proceeding from a luminous point to a focal point on the retina, barring the disturbing influence of chromatic and spherical aberration. If, however, one or all of the refractive surfaces have unequal curvatures along different merid- ians, then it is obvious that the rays from a luminous point can not be brought to a focal point, since the rays along the meridian of greater curvature will be brought to a focus first and begin to diverge before the rays along the lesser curvature are focused. Such a condition is designated as astigmatism (from a, not, and auyfxa, point). The effect may be illustrated by the diagram in Fig. 126, which represents the refraction of the rays from a luminous point by a planoconvex lens whose curvature along the vertical meridian is greater than along the horizontal meridian. The rays along the vertical meridian are brought to a focus first at (r,but those from the horizontal meridian are still converging; so that a screen placed at this point will give the image of a horizontal line (a-ar). The rays along the horizontal meridian are brought to a focus at B, but those from focus G have by this time spread out in a vertical plane, so that a screen placed at this point will give the image of a vertical line (b-c). In between the images will be elliptical or circular, as represented in the diagram. In the eye astigmatism may be due to an inequality in curvature of either the cornea or the lens, and may be either regular or irregular. By regular astigmatism is meant that condition in which while the curvature along each individual meridian is equal throughout its course, the curvatures of the different meridians vary and in such 298 THE SPECIAL SENSES. a way that the meridians of greatest and least curvature are at right angles to each other. Ordinary astigmatism is of the regular variety, and is usually attributed to a defect in the curvature of the cornea. If the astigmatism is such that the vertical meridian has the greatest curvature it is termed "with the rule," since normally this meridian is slightly more curved than the horizontal one. If, on the contrary, the curvature along the horizontal meridian is greater, the astigmatism is " against the rule." The meridians of greatest and least curvature may not lie in the vertical and horizontal planes, but in some of the oblique planes; but so long as they are at right angles the astigmatism is regular. It is evident that such a condition may be corrected by the use of cylindrical Fig. 126. — Schema to illustrate the paths of the rays of light in a cornea showing regular astigmatism. — (McKendrick.) The lower line of figures represents the section of the cone of light, or the images obtained at different distances. The image varies from a. horizontal to a vertical line, but at no place can a point be obtained at which rays along all meridians are focused. glasses so chosen as to increase the curvature along the meridian in which the cornea has the least curvature. An eye that suffers from a marked degree of astigmatism can not focus distinctly at the same time lines that are at right angles to each other; hence the use of a series of lines whose rays fall along the different meridians of the cornea, as shown in Fig. 127, will reveal this defect if it exists. If the eye is directed to the center of intersection of the lines some of the lines appear distinct while those at right angles to them are blurred. A normal eye can be thrown into an astigmatic con- dition by approximating the eyelids closely. In this position the tears make a concave cylindrical lens, which alters the curvature along the vertical meridian. What is known as irregular astig- matism is due to the fact that the meridians of greatest and least DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 299 ^^ curvature are not at approximately right angles, or, as is more commonly the case, it is due to an irregularity in the curvature along some one meridian, such as may be produced by a scar upon the cornea. This condition may be produced from a variety of causes affecting either the cornea or the lens, and practically it can not be corrected by the use of lenses. As Helmholtz has shown, a small degree of irregular astigmatism is present nor- mally, owing to a certain asymmetry in the curvature of the lens. This defect is made apparent in the visual sensations caused by a point of light, such as is furnished, for instance, by a fixed star. The retinal image in these cases, instead of being a sym- metrical point, is a radiate figure the exact form of which may vary in different eyes. For this reason the fixed stars give us the well- known star-shaped image instead of a clearly defined point. Innervation and Physiological Control of the Ciliary Muscle and the Muscles of the Iris. — From an optical point of view the iris plays the part of a diaphragm. It is, moreover, an adjustable diaphragm the aperture of which — that is, the size of the pupil — is varied reflexly according to the conditions of illumination. Its adjustments are made possible by the fact that it contains within its substance two bands of muscular tissue, one, the sphincter muscle, forming a circular ring whose contraction diminishes the aperture of the pupil, and the other a dilator muscle whose contrac- tion widens the pupil. Each of these muscles possesses its own nerve fibers that arise ultimately from the brain, and through these fibers reflex movements of great delicacy are effected. The sphinc- ter pupillse is a well-defined band of plain muscle whose width varies, according to the state of contraction, from 0.6 to 1.2 mms.; it forms a ring lying just on the margin of the pupil, and it is im- bedded in the stroma of the iris. The histological differentiation of the dilator pupillse is much less distinct. For a long time its existence was the subject of controversy, but it is now conceded that such a muscle is present in the form of a layer of elongated spindle-like cells which lie close to the pigment layer of the iris and Fig. 127. — Astigmatic chart. 300 THE SPECIAL SENSES. form radial bundles stretching from the ciliary border of the iris toward the pupillary orifice.* Both of these muscles are supplied by autonomic nerve fibers — that is, the motor nerve path comprises a preganglionic fiber, arising from the central nervous system, and a postganglionic fiber, arising from a sympathetic ganglion. Anatomically it can be shown that the sphincter muscle is supplied by the short ciliary nerves arising from the ciliary ganglion, which supply also the muscle of accommoda- tion, the ciliary muscle; while the dilator muscle is supplied by the long ciliary nerves that arise from the ophthalmic branch of the fifth cra- nial nerve, as represented in Fig. 128. The entire course of the motor paths, preganglionic and postganglionic fibers, is represented diagrammat- ically in Fig. 129. The motor fibers to the ciliary muscle and sphincter pupil lae arise in the mid- brain in the nucleus of origin of the third cranial nerve, and indeed in a special part of this nu- cleus lying most ante- riorly. They leave the third nerve in the orbit and end within the sub- stance of the ciliary gan- glion, whence the path is continued by sympa- thetic ( postganglionic ) fibers emerging from the ganglion in the short ciliary nerves. The fibers to the dilator muscle have a very different path. They arise also in the brain, most probably in the midbrain, although their exact origin has not been determined satisfactorily, and pass down the spinal Course of constrictor nerve fibers, Course of dilator nerve fibers, Fig. 128. — Diagrammatic representation of the nerves governing the pupil (after Foster) : II, Optic nerve; l.g, ciliary ganglion; r.b, its short root from ///, motor oculi nerve; sym., its sympathetic root; rl, its long root from V, ophthalmonasal branch of oph- thalmic division of fifth nerve; s.c, short ciliary nerves; l.c, long ciliary nerves. * For a physiological proof and the literature of the controversy see Langley and Anderson, "Journal of Physiology/' 13, 554, 1892. For the histological proof, Grunert, "Archives of Ophthalmology," 30, 377, 1901. DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 301 cord to terminate in the lower cervical region. From this point the path is continued by spinal neurons which leave the cord in the eighth cervical and the first and second thoracic spinal nerves and pass by way of the corresponding rami communicantes into the sympathetic chain at the level of the first thoracic ganglion. From this point the fibers pass upward in the cervical sympathetic with- out terminating until they reach the superior cervical ganglion near the base of the skull. From this ganglion the path is continued by sympathetic (postganglionic) fibers which pass to the Gasserian ganglion and unite with its ophthalmic branch. Subsequently they leave the ophthalmic nerve in the long ciliary branches. These fibers under normal conditions are in constant (tonic) activity, so that if the path is interrupted at any point by section of the cervical Gasserian Oangllon.. Superior Cervical. pnq ciiiaru nerves. fianqlii Dilator jDupillae. tyhitieter /oupillae. yhinal Cord. CihuyGaytiau Jhorf CdiapyTwvtt^ 0f Ca//T0/v^ iewical ( jsSSS^ — ^T /x HCD.^V Fig. 129. — Schema showing the path of the preganglionic and postgaiignuiim mrers to the ciliary muscle and to the sphincter and dilator muscles of the iris. — (Modified from Schultz.) The course of the long ciliary nerves is represented very diagrammatically. sympathetic, for instance — the pupil is seen to contract. This constant activity may be referred directly to the activity of the spinal neurons whose cells lie in the spina] cord in the lower cervical and upper thoracic region. The cells in question constitute what is sometimes called the lower ciliospinal center of Budge. The Accommodation Reflex and the Light Reflex of the Sphincter Muscle. — When the eye is accommodated for a near object by the contraction of the ciliary muscle there is always a simultaneous contraction of the sphincter pupillae whereby the pupil is narrowed. The act is one of obvious value in vision, since by diaphragming down the lens the focus is improved and more exact vision, such as is needed in close work, is obtained. The act is usually spoken of as the accommodation reflex, but in reality it A 302 THE SPECIAL SENSES. is rather what isjmown as an associated movement. The voluntary ejffpijt inattgyi*^ted in the brain affects the cranial centers for both "" rl'iuyi'les,' and under normal conditions they always act together, — a fact which implies a close connection of their centers. An example of a similar associated action is seen in the effect of the respiratory movements on the rate of heart beat, the inspiratory discharge from the respiratory center being accompanied by an associated effect upon the cardio-inhibitory center whereby the heart rate is quickened. In the particular case that we are dealing with three muscular acts, in fact, are usually associated, for every act of accommodation under normal circumstances is accompanied not only by a constriction of the pupil, but also by a convergence of the eyeballs, due to a contraction of the internal rectus muscle in each eye. The light reflex is observed when light is thrown into the eye. As is well known, the pupil dilates in darkness or dim lights and contracts to a pin-point upon strong illumination of the retina. The value of this reflex is also obvious. In the dim light the total illumination and therefore the visual power of the retina is aided by an enlarged pupil, but in strong lights the illumination may be diminished with advantage by diaphragming, since the optical image on the retina is thereby improved on account of the diminu- tion in spherical aberration. The reflex arc involved in this act is known in part. The afferent path is along the optic nerve; the efferent path back to the sphincter is through the third nerve and ciliary ganglion ; injury to either of these paths diminishes or destroys the reflex. The reflex is also lost in some cases in which neither of these paths seems to be involved. In tabes dorsalis (locomotor ataxia) and general paresis, for instance, the pupil of the eye is constricted and does not give the light reflex, but still shows the accommodation reflex. Such a condition is known as the Argyll Robertson pupil. Some question exists, there- fore, as to the nature of the connections in the brain between the afferent impulses and the motor center in the nucleus of the third nerve. According to some authors (Gudden, v. Bechterew), the afferent light reflex fibers are a set of fibers distinct from the visual fibers proper. They arise in the retina and pass backward in the optic nerve, but leave the optic tracts at the chiasma to enter the walls of the third ventricle and thus reach the nucleus of the third nerve. This view, however, finds no support in the histological structure of the retina. Under normal conditions the light reflex is bilateral, — that is, light thrown upon one retina only will cause constriction of the pupil in both eyes. In those of the lower ani- mals whose optic nerves cross completely in the chiasma the light reflex, on the contrary, is unilateral, affecting only the eye that ( DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. V fi 303 is stimulated.* We may conclude, therefore, that thebHo^ilii^ tJpaftt of the reflex in the higher animals is dependent upon the partial decussation of the optic fibers in the chiasma, a sensory stimulus upon one retina giving rise to impulses which are conveyed to the two sides of the brain. It is possible, however, that in addition commissural connections may exist between the central connections, — the motor centers in the midbrain. It is usually stated that the effect of the light upon the sphincter muscle is greatest when the retina is stimulated at or near the fovea and that it varies directly with the intensity of the light and the area illuminated.t The Action of Drugs upon the Iris. — The condition of con- striction of the pupil is frequently designated as miosis (mi-o'-sis) and the condition of dilatation as mydriasis (myd-ri'-as-is). Many drugs are known which, when applied directly to the absorptive surfaces of the eye or when injected into the circulation, affect the muscles of the iris and therefore vary the size of the pupil. Those drugs that cause miosis are spoken of as miotics, and those that produce mydriasis as mydriatics. Atropin, the active prin- ciple of belladonna, homatropin, and cocain are well-known myd- riatics, while physostigmin (eserin) and muscarin or pilocarpin are examples of the miotics. There has been much question as to the precise action of these drugs. For an adequate discussion of this question the studenf is referred to works on pharmacology; but it may be said that the evidence from the physiological sidej indi- cates that atropin causes mydriasis by paralyzing the endings of the constrictor nerve fibers in the sphincter muscle, while phy- sostigmin and muscarin cause miosis by stimulation of the endings of these same fibers. In the case of cocain it is probable that the drug first stimulates mainly the endings of the dilator fibers in the dilator muscles, and in stronger doses causes additional mydriasis by paralyzing the constrictor fibers. The stronger mydriatics paralyze not only the sphincter pupillse, but also the similarly innervated ciliary muscle, thus destroying the power of accom- modation. When atropin is applied to the eye the individual is unable to use his eyes for near work — reading, for example — until the effect of the drug has worn off. In ophthalmological literature this condition of paralysis of the ciliary muscle is spoken of as cycloplegia, and most of the mydriatic drugs are also cycloplegics. On the contrary, the stronger miotics stimulate the ciliary muscle, and therefore during their period of action throw the eye into a condition of forced accommodation. * Steinach, "Archiv f. d. gesammte Physiologie," 47, 313, 1890. fSee Abelsdorff and Feilchenfell, "Zeitschrift f. Psychologie und Phys- iologie des Sinnesorgane," 34, 111, 1904. % Schultz, "Archiv f. Physiologie," 1898, 47. 304 THE SPECIAL SENSES. In the above description of the innervation of the iris and the causes of mydriasis and miosis the simplest explanations offered have been adopted. It should be added, however, that some facts are known which indicate that the conditions are more complex. Thus, Meltzer and Auer* have shown that in mammals the application of solutions of adrenalin to the eye has no effect on the iris and the same is true after section of the cervical sympathetic. But if the superior cervical ganglion is removed the adrenalin causes a maxi- mal mydriasis. This paradoxical dilatation indicates that the ganglion has some specific influence upon the iris in addition to serving as part of the pathway for the pupillodilator fibers, since as long as it is present it prevents the adrenalin from acting upon the musculature of the iris. The Balanced Action of the Sphincter and Dilator Muscles of the Iris. — It would seem that under normal conditions both the sphincter and the dilator muscle are kept more or less in tonic activity by impulses received through their respective motor fibers. They thus balance each other, to speak figuratively, and a mechan- ism of this kind in which two opposing actions are in play is in a condition to respond promptly and smoothly to an excess of stimu- lation from either side. The two muscles, in fact, act as antago- nists in the same manner as the flexor and extensor muscles around a joint. At the same time this relation adds some difficulties to the explanation of specific reactions, since it is evident that a dila- tation of the pupil may be caused either by a contraction of the dilator muscle or a loss of tone (inhibition) in the sphincter, while in constriction of the pupil the effect may resuit either from a con- traction of the sphincter or an inhibition of the dilator; or, last, the contraction of one muscle may always be accompanied by an inhibition of its antagonist, as is supposed to be the case with the flexor and extensor muscles of the limbs. Andersonf has given some evidence to show that the dilatation of the pupil in cats is due to a double action of this sort, the pupillodilator muscle con- tracting first and subsequently the tone of the constrictors suf- fering an inhibition. Alterations in the size of the pupil take place not only under the conditions described above — namely, the light and the accommodation reflex and the action of drugs, — but also under many other circumstances, normal and pathological. In sleep, for instance, the eyes roll upward and inward and the pupils are constricted. It would seem probable that the miosis in this case is due to a cessation in tonic activity on the part of the dilator muscle rather than to an active contraction of the sphincter muscle, the state of sleep being characterized by a diminution in activity in the central nervous system. Emotional states also affect the size of the pupil and thus aid in giving the facial expressions char- acteristic of these conditions. Writers speak of the eyes dilating with terror or darkening with emotions of deep pleasure. This pupil- * Meltzer and Auer, "American Journal of Physiology," 11, 28, and 40, 1904. t " Journal of Physiology," 30, 15, 1903. DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 305 lary accompaniment of the emotional states may occur even when it is a matter of memory rather than immediate experience. The explanation of this mydriasis can hard]y be obtained by experi- ment, but reasoning from analogy we know that strong emotional states are usually accompanied by more or less distinct inhibitory effects on motor centers, and perhaps in this case the reaction is most satisfactorily explained by attributing it to an inhibition of the constrictor center in the midbrain. The Ophthalmoscope. — The light that falls into the eye is partly absorbed by the black pig- ment of the choroid coat and is partly reflected back to the ex- terior. This latter portion is re- flected back in the direction in which it entered. Merely holding a light near the eye does not, there- fore, enable us to see the interior more clearly, since in order to catch the returning rays in our own eye it would be necessary to interpose the head between the source of light and the observed eye. If, however, we could arrange the light to enter the observed eye as though it pro- ceeded from our own eye, then the returning rays would be perceived, and with sufficient illumination the bottom or fundus of the observed eye might be seen. Arguing in this way, Helmholtz constructed his first form of the ophthalmoscope in 1851. The value of the ophthalmoscope is twofold: It enables the observer to examine the interior of the eye and thus recognize diseased conditions of the retina ; it is also useful in detect- ing abnormalities in the refractive surfaces of the eye. The principle of the instrument is well repre- sented in the original form devised by Helmholtz, as shown schemat- ically in Fig. 131, A . I represents the observed eye and 77 the eye of the observer. Between the two eyes is placed a piece of glass inclined at an angle. Light from the candle falling upon this glass is in part reflected from the surface to enter eye I, and these rays on emerging from the eye along the same line pass through the glass in part and enter eve 77. In place of the plane unsilvered glass it is now cus- "20 Loring's ophthalmoscope. 306 THE SPECIAL SENSES. tomary to use a concave mirror with a small hole through the center, the observer's eye being placed directly behind this hole. Such an instrument is shown in Fig. 130. The instrument is used in two ways, known as the direct and the indirect method. In the direct method the mirror is held very close to the observed eye and the paths of the rays of light into and out of the eye are represented schematically in Fig. 131, B. The light from a lamp caught upon the Fig. 131. — Diagrams to represent the principle of the ophthalmoscope: A, The orig- inal form of ophthalmoscope, consisting of a piece of glass, M, inclined at a suitable angle The rays from the source of light are reflected into the observed eye, /, and thence return along the same lines passing through M to reach the observer's eye, II. B, The direct method with the ophthalmoscopic mirror. The rays of light illuminate the fundus of the observed eye, /, and thence pass out in parallel rays, if the eye is emmetropic, to reach the observer's eye, //. C, the indirect method with ophthalmoscopic mirror and intercalated lens. The rays of light-red lines are brought to a focus within the anterior chamber of the eye and thence diverge to give a general illumination of the interior of the eyeball. The returning rays of light are indicated for a single point, b. At a', &', c', a real inverted image of a portion of the retina is formed in the air, which in turn is focused on the retina of the observer's eye. mirror is thrown into the eye, the rays coming to a focus and then spreading out so as to give a diffuse illumination of the fundus. This latter surface may now be considered as a luminous object sending out rays of light. Taking any three objects on the retina, A, B, C, it is apparent that if eye / is an emmetropic eye these points are at the principal focal distance and the rays sent from each after emerging from the eye are in parallel bundles. These rays penetrate the hole in the mirror and fall into the observer's DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 307 eye as though they came from distant objects. If the observer's eye is also emmetropic, or is made so by suitable glasses, these bundles of rays will be focused on his retina without an act of accom- modation. He must, in fact, in looking through the mirror gaze, not at the eye before him, but, relaxing his accommodation, gaze through the eye, as it were, into the distance. In this way he will see the portion of the retina illuminated, the image of the objects seen being inverted on his own retina and therefore projected or seen erect. If the observed eye is myopic its retina is farther back than the principal focus of its refracting surfaces; consequently the rays sent out from the illuminated retina emerge in converging bundles and can not be focused on the retina of the observer's eye. By inserting a concave lens of proper power between his eye and the mirror the observer can render the rays parallel and thus bring out the image. From the power of the lens used the degree of my- opia may be estimated. Just the reverse happens if the observed eye is hypermetropic. In such an eye the retina is nearer than the principal focal distance of the refractive surface ; consequently the light emitted from the retina emerges in bundles of diverging rays which cannot be brought to a focus on the retina of the ob- server unless he exerts his own power of accommodation or inter- poses a convex lens between his eye and the mirror. The indirect method of using the ophthalmoscope is represented in Fig. 131, C. The mirror is held at some distance, at arm's length, from the observed eye, /, while just before this eye a biconvex lens of short focus is placed. As shown in the diagram by the red lines, the reflected light from the mirror comes to a focus and then diverging falls upon the biconvex lens. This lens brings the rays to a focus at or in the eye, whence they again diverge and light up the retina with a diffuse illumination. The light from this retina is in turn sent back toward the mirror, its path being indicated for the point b by the black lines. If the eye is emmetropic the rays from this point emerge parallel, and falling upon the biconvex lens are brought to a focus at b'. Similarly the rays from a will be brought to a focus at a' and from c at c'. Consequently there will be formed in the air an inverted image, and it is at this image that the eye of the observer gazes through the hole in the mirror. This image forms its image on the retina of the observer's eye, as repre- sented in the diagram at a", b", c", and is projected outward or seen inverted as regards the original position of the points in the retina of eye /. The indirect method is the one usually employed in ophthalmoscopic examinations of the retina. It gives a larger field than the direct method, although the objects seen are of smaller size. CHAPTER XVIII. THE PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA— VISUAL STIMULI AND VISUAL SENSATIONS. The Portion of the Retina Stimulated by Light. — The normal stimulus to the sensory cells in the retina is found in the vibrations of the ether, the waves of light. When sunlight is passed through a prism the waves of different lengths are dispersed, and those capable of stimulating the retina form the visible spectrum extending from red to violet. The limits of the spectrum are, on the one hand, the extreme red rays with a wave length of xrrlinnr mm- and vibrating at the rate of about 390,000,000,000,000 a second, and, on the other, the extreme violet, having a wave length of about -nHnFihns" mm- ano^ a rate of vibration of 757,000,000,000,000 a second. The part of the retina stimulated by these vibrations is supposed to be the layer Of rods and cones. To reach these structures the light must pass Fig. 132. — To demonstrate the blind spot. Fix the center of the cross with the right eye, then move the book slowly to or from the face. At a certain distance the image of the large circle to the right will disappear. At this distance the image of the circle falls on the optic disc. through the other layers of the retina. That the rods and cones are the structures that react to the light stimulation is indicated by their structure and their connections and by such facts as the follow- ing: Under certain conditions, which are described below, the shadows of the retinal vessels and the contained corpuscles may be seen, a fact which indicates that the perceiving structures lie ex- ternally to these vessels. In the fovea centralis, in which vision is most perfect, the layers of the retina are thinned out until practically only the rods and cones remain to be acted upon. That the optic nerve fibers themselves are not acted upon by light waves is proved by the existence of the blind spot. The termination of the optic nerve within the eyeball, the optic disc, lies about 15 degrees to the nasal side of the fovea and has a diameter of about 1.5 mms. From 308 PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 309 this point the nerve fibers spread out over the rest of the optic cup to form the internal layer of the retina. But the optic disc itself has no retinal structure, and light that falls upon it is not perceived. The presence of this blind spot in our visual field is easily demon- strated by the experiment illustrated and described in Fig. 132. In the visual field for each eye, therefore, there is a gap representing the projection of the area of the optic disc to the exterior, the size of the gap increasing with the distance from the eye. We do not notice this deficiency, inasmuch as it exists in our indirect field of vision (see below), in which our perception of form is poorly de- veloped; so that any disturbance in outline that might result in the retinal image of external objects is unperceived. Morever, the portion of the external world that falls on the blind spot of one eye falls on the retinal field of the other, and is thus perceived in binoc- ular vision. It is to be borne in mind, also, that the projection of the blind spot does not appear in the visual field as a dark area ; it is simply an absent area, so that no gap exists in our consciousness of the spatial relations of the visual field ; the margins, so to speak, of the hole come into contact so far as our consciousness is concerned. The Action Current Caused by Stimulation of the Retina. — The effect of light waves falling upon the retina is to set up a series of nerve impulses in the optic nerve fibers. It is interesting to find that these impulses aroused in a sensory nerve by a normal stimulus are attended by electrical changes similar to those observed in motor fibers when stimulated normally or artificially. The fact strengthens the view that the electrical change is an invariable ac- companiment of the nerve impulse, if not the nerve impulse itself. If the eye is excised and connected with a galvanometer or capillary electrometer by two non-polarizable electrodes, one placed upon the cut end of the optic nerve and the other on the cornea, the usual demarcation current is obtained due to the injury to the optic nerve. If the preparation is kept in the dark and arrangements are made to throw a light through the pupil upon the retina the galvanometer indicates an electrical change or current whenever the light is admitted.* The direction of the current in the eyeball is from the fundus to the cornea, and as regards the pre-existing demarcation current it is in the same direction and forms, therefore, a so-called positive variation. When the electrodes are placed on the longi- tudinal and the cut surface of the optic nerve, then, according to Kuhne, the electrical response to light is a negative variation similar to that described for stimulation of nerves in general (p. 96). Not only is there a "light response" each time that the retina is stimu- lated by light, but there is a similar electrical change, a "dark re- * Dewar and McKendrick, " Transactions, Royal Society, Edinburgh," 27, 1873; Gotch, "Journal of Physiology," 29, 388, 1903, and 31, 1, 1904. 310 THE SPECIAL SENSES. sponse," when the light is suddenly withdrawn. This last interest- ing fact would seem to indicate a stimulation process of some kind in the retina due to darkness, — that is, withdrawal of the objective stimulus. The reaction is a very sensitive one, lights so weak as to be near the threshold for the human eye give a distinct electrical change in the frog's retina, and an eye that has been kept in the dark for some time (dark-adapted eye) shows an increased sensitive- ness. It is very interesting, also, to find that the frog's retina responds to a range of light vibrations that corresponds with the limits of the visible spectrum as seen by the human eye. If the electrical response is a true indication of functional activity it would appear that the frog's vision has about the same extent as our own as regards the ether waves of different periods of vibration. The Visual Purple— Rhodopsin. — The change that takes place in the rods and cones whereby the vibratory energy of the ether waves is converted into nerve impulses is unknown. It has been assumed by some observers that the light waves act mechanically, the wave movements setting into vibration portions of the external segments of the rods or cones, and that this mechanical movement forms the direct excitant of the nerve impulses.* The general view, however, is that the process is photochemical, — that is, the impact of the ether waves sets up chemical changes in the rods or cones which in turn give rise to nerve impulses that are transmitted to the brain. We have an analogy for this action in the known change produced by light upon sensitized photographic films. In the retina itself some basis for such a view is found in the existence of a red pigment which is bleached by light. This interesting dis- covery was made by Boll,| and the facts were afterward carefully investigated by Kuhne.t The red pigment, known usually as visual purple or rhodopsin, is found only in the external segments of the rods; the cones do not contain it. In the fovea, therefore, which has only cones, the pigment is entirely absent. The existence of the visual purple may be demonstrated very easily. A frog is kept for some time in the dark; it is then killed and an eye removed and bisected equatorially. If the vitreous is removed from the pos- terior half the retina may be detached by means of a pair of forceps. When the operation is performed in red or yellow light, as in photo- graphic work, the detached retina on examination by daylight is found to be a deep-red color; but after a short exposure it fades rapidly, finally becoming colorless. If the frogs before operation were exposed to strong daylight, the retina is found to be * Zenker, "Archiv f. mik. Anatomie," 3, 248, 1867. fBoll, "Archiv f. Physiologie," 1877, 4. j Kiihne, "Untersuch. a. d. physiol. Inst. d. Univ. Heidelberg," vol. i, 1878. Also "The Photochemistry of the Retina, etc.," translated by Foster, London, 1878. PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 311 colorless. A similar pigment is found in the eyes of man and the other mammalia. It has been shown, moreover, that a photo- graph may be made upon the surface of the retina by means of this purple. If the head of a rabbit or frog that has been kept in the dark for some time is exposed with proper precautions to the light of a window, for instance, the part of the retina on which the image of the window-lights falls will be bleached, while the parts upon which the image of the window-bars falls and the surrounding areas of the retina will retain their red color. A figure of such a retinal photograph or optogram, as it is called, is represented in the accom- panying illustration (Fig. 133) . The visual purple has been extracted from the rods by solutions of bile salts, this substance having the power to discharge the pigment from its combination in the rods in the same way as it discharges hemoglobin from its combination in the red corpuscles. The solutions thus obtained are also bleached upon exposure to light. We have in the visual purple, therefore, an unstable substance readily decomposed or altered by the me- chanical effect of the ether waves, and also, it may be said, by gross mechanical re- actions, such as com- pression; and there Fig 133_ Optogram in eye of rabbit: 1, The nor- Can be little doubt ma^ appearance of the retina in the rabbit's eye: a, The entrance of the optic nerve; b, b, a colorless strip of that the Substance medullated nerve fibers; c, a strip of deeper color sepa- , . rating the lighter upper from the more heavily pigmented plays an important lower portion. 2 shows the optogram of a window. part in the functional response of the rod elements. It has been shown that provision exists in the retina for the constant regeneration of this red pigment. It will be remembered that the external segments of the rods im- pinge upon the heavily pigmented epithelial cells that lie between the rods and the choroid coat. From experiments upon frogs' eyes it appears that a portion of the retina detached from the pigment cells and bleached by the action of light is not able to regenerate its visual purple until again laid back upon the choroid coat. This regenerating influence of the black pigmented cells may be con- nected with another interesting relation that they exhibit. Under normal conditions delicate processes extend from these cells and penetrate between the rods and cones. When the eye is exposed to light the black pigment migrates along these processes as far even as the external limiting membrane, and it is possible that this ar- rangement may be useful in obviating diffuse radiation of light from one rod to another. When the eye is kept in the dark, however, 312 THE SPECIAL SENSES. the pigment moves outwardly and collects around the external segments, where the process of regeneration of the visual purple is taking place. Further evidence that the visual purple is connected with the irritability of the rods toward light stimulation is shown by the fact that when it is exposed to the different rays of the spec- trum the absorption of light is greatest in that part of the spec- trum (green) which appears the brightest in vision when carried out under such conditions as may be supposed to involve the activity chiefly of the rods (see below for these conditions). It is, however, perfectly obvious that visual purple is not essential to vision. The fact that it is absent from the fovea centralis is alone sufficient proof of this statement. Moreover, it seems to be absent entirely in the eyes of some animals; for instance, the pigeon, hen, some reptiles, and some bats. The most attractive view of the function of the visual purple is that it serves to increase the delicacy of re- sponse or irritability of the rods in dim lights, — a view that is ex- plained in more detail in the paragraph below, dealing with the sup- posed difference in function between the rods and cones. The Extent of the Visual Field — Perimetry. — By the visual field of each eye is meant the entire extent of the external world which when the eye is fixed forms an image upon or is projected upon the retina of that eye. From what has been said previously regarding the dioptrics of the eye it is obvious that the visual field is inverted upon the retina, and that, therefore, objects in the upper visual field fall upon the lower half of the retina, and objects in the right half of the visual field fall upon the left half of the retina. Since the retina is sensitive to light up to the ora serrata, it is evident that if the eye were protruded sufficiently from its orbit its projected visual field when represented upon a flat surface would have the form of a circle the center of which would correspond to the fovea centralis. As a matter of fact, the configuration of the face is such as to cut off a considerable part of this field and to give to the field as it actually exists an irregular outline. The bridge of the nose, the projecting eyebrows and cheek bones serve to thus limit the field. To obtain the exact outline and extent of the visual field in any given case it is only necessary to keep the eye fixed and then to move a small object in the different meridians and at the same distance from the eye. The limits of vision may be obtained in this way along each meridian and the results combined upon an appropriate chart. An instrument, the perimeter, has been devised to facilitate the process of charting the visual field. It has been given a number of different forms, one of which is illustrated in Fig. 134. The shape of the visual fields in the normal eye is represented in Fig. 135. The determination of the visual fields is of especial importance in cases of brain lesions involving the visual area in the occipital lobe. PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 313 The extent and portion of the retina affected may be used to aid in locating the seat of the lesion. For physiological and for clinical purposes it is necessary to distinguish between the central (or direct) and the peripheral (or indirect) fields of vision. The former term is meant to refer to that portion of the field which falls upon the fovea centralis; in other words, it is the projection, in any fixed Fig. 134. — Perimeter. The semicircular bar may be placed in any meridian. A given object is then moved along the bar from without in until it is just perceived. The angular distance at which this occurs is marked off on the corresponding meridian on the chart seen at the left of the figure. The eye examined gazes over the top of the vertical rod at the right at a fixed point in the middle of the semicircular bar. position of the eye, of the fovea into the external world. The peripheral field refers to the rest of the visual field involving the retina outside the fovea. As a matter of fact, all of our distinct and most useful vision in the daytime at least is effected through the fovea. When the eye is kept fixed the small portion of the external world that falls upon the fovea is seen distinctly. All the rest is seen more or less indistinctly in proportion to the distance of its 314 THE SPECIAL SENSES. retinal image from the fovea. In using our eyes, therefore, we keep them continually in motion so as to bring each object, as we pay especial attention to it, in the field of centra] vision. The line from the fovea to the point looked at is designated as the line of sight. The area of the fovea is quite small. The measurements given by different observers vary somewhat, especially as in some cases the measurements are estimated for the bottom of the de- pression, the fundus, and in others for the diameter from edge to edge. The average diameter is usually given as lying between 0.3 and 0.4 mm. Lines drawn from the ends of this diameter to the Fig. 135. -Perimeter chart to show the field of vision for a right eye when kept in a fixed position. nodal point of the eye subtend an angle of 1 degree to 1.5 degrees; and therefore all objects in the external world around the line of sight whose visual angle is within this limit are comprised in the central field of vision, and their retinal images fall upon the fovea. Unilateral lesions of one occipital lobe cause half-blindness (hemi- opia) in the retinas on the same side, — that is, lesions in the right occipital lobe cause blindness of the right halves of the retinas, while injuries to the left occipital lobes are accompanied by loss of vision on the left sides of the retinas (see p. 193); but such unilateral lesions, it is stated, do not involve the central field of vision — only the peripheral portion of the field is affected. In connection with PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 315 its special functions in vision the fovea centralis possesses a peculiar structure. It forms a shallow depression in the center of the retina described by some authors as elliptical, by others as circular in out- line. In the center of the fovea lies a smaller, very shallow depres- sion spoken of as the foveola. The diameter of the fovea, as stated above, is estimated differently by different authors. While meas- urements on preserved specimens give the diameter as 0.2 to 0.4 mm., ophthalmoscopic examination seems to indicate that in the fresh state it may be larger. According to Fritsch,* the fundus, reckoned from the point at which the depression begins, has a diam- eter of 0.5 to 0.75 mm. Within the fovea cones only are present, and these cones are longer and more slender (diameter, 0.002 mm.) than in the rest of the retina. Moreover, the thickness of the retina is much reduced in the fovea, whence arises the depression. At this point the cones are practically exposed directly to the light, whereas in other parts the light must penetrate the other layers before reaching the rods and cones. Lying around the fovea is an area about 6 mm. in diameter, of a yellowish color, and hence known as the macula lutea. Central vision is sometimes designated erroneously as macular vision instead of foveal vision. Visual Acuity. — The distinctness of vision varies greatly in different parts of the retina. It is usually measured by bringing two fine lines closer and closer together until the eye is unable to see them as two distinct objects. Measured in this way it is usually stated that when the distance between the lines subtends an angle of 1 minute (60 seconds) at the eye the limit of visibility is reached. This angle on the retina comprises an area of about 0.004 mm. in diameter, sufficient to cover two cones in the fovea. A simpler method to ascertain the size of a just perceptible image on the retina is to use a black spot upon a white background. At a sufficient distance this object will be invisible, but if brought closer to the eye it will be just seen at a certain distance. The diameter of the spot being known, and its distance from the eye, the size of the retinal image may be calculated. Using this method, Guillery estimated the size of the just perceptible retinal image, or, as it has been appropriately called, the 'physiological point, at 0.0035 mm. These estimates apply only to the fovea, and, indeed, to the central part of the fovea, the foveola. Numerous authors have called attention to the fact that the size of the physiological point for the fovea varies with the intensity of illumination. The estimates given are for ordinary room light. Out of doors, and especially in the case of persons who live habitually an outdoor life, the physio- * Fritsch, " Sitzungsberichte d. konig. Akad. d. Wiss.," Berlin, 1900. t Guillery, "Zeitschrift f. Psychologie u. Physiol, d. Sinnesorgane," 12, 243, 1896. 316 THE SPECIAL SENSES. logical point is smaller — less than half the size given above. We may believe, therefore, that under the most favorable conditions we can perceive an object whose image on the fovea is less than the diameter (0.002 mm.) of a single cone. The acuity of vision does not vary greatly throughout the fovea; any object whose retinal image falls well within the fovea can be seen quite distinctly in all of its parts when the eye is fixed for the center of the object. This If ■ir fr cv T^ / / \ / \ / i \ / ^ / s s s r — ___ s»- — -- >>. s — - — — -^ *"< — - — \ s €0* 30° 40 30 £0° HT4m' 1T"4r Hf ZO9 J0° W <50* 60° Fig. 136. — Curve to show the relative acuity of vision in the central and peripheral fields and in the light-adapted and the dark-adapted eye. — (Koester.) The full line repre- sents the relative acuteness of vision in the eye exposed to usual illumination. From the center of the fovea, 0°, the acuity of vision falls rapidly at first and then more slowly as one passes outward into the peripheral field. The dotted line represents the acuity of vision in dim lights. The fovea, it will be noticed, is less sensitive than the parts of the retina at an angular distance of 10° or even 60°. is the case, for instance, with the moon. Nevertheless, in looking at such an object as the moon the eye to make out details will fixate one point after another, showing that for most distinct vision we use probably only the center of the fovea. As we pass out from the fovea in the peripheral field of vision the acuity of vision diminishes at first very rapidly, so that at 20 degrees, for instance, from the center of the fovea the physiological point on the retina is 0.035 mm.; that is, it has a diameter ten times as large as in the PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 317 fovea. On this account our vision in the peripheral field is very- indistinct, — details of form cannot be clearly perceived. The rapidity with which visual acuity diminishes as we pass outward from the fovea is indicated by the curve given in Fig. 136. In all close work, therefore, we keep our eyes moving continually so as to bring one point after another into the center of the fovea, as is well illustrated by the act of reading. If the eye is kept fixed upon the central letter of a long word, only one or two letters on each side can be made out distinctly in spite of the fact that with such familiar objects we can guess the letter even when the image is not entirely distinct. In ophthalmological practice the acuity of vision (central vision) is measured usually by test letters whose size is such that at the distance at which they are read — say, 6 meters (20 feet), the practical far point at which no accommodation is needed — each subtends at the eye an angle of 5 minutes. An eye that can distinguish the letters at this distance is said to be normal ; one that can distinguish them only at a smaller distance or at the given distance requires letters of larger size has a subnormal acuity of vision. If, for instance, an individual at 20 feet can read only those letters that the normal eye can distinguish at 100 feet his visual acuity, V, is equal to y%V Relation between the Amount of Sensation and the Intensity of the Stimulus — Threshold Stimulus. — With the sensory as with the motor nerves we may distinguish between various degrees of sub- maximal stimulation. The stronger the stimulus, the stronger the reaction, — that is, in the case of the optic nerve, the visual sensation. The end reaction of the activity of a sensory nerve is a state of con- sciousness. The variations in magnitude of this state can not be measured with objective exactness, they must be judged subjectively by the individual concerned. A stimulus too weak to give a re- sponse with a motor nerve is usually designated in physiology as subminimal ; a similar stimulus with sensory nerves is frequently expressed by the equivalent term subliminal, — that is, below the threshold. So a stimulus just strong enough to provoke a percep- tible reaction is the minimal stimulus for efferent nerves and the threshold stimulus for sensory nerves. Inasmuch as the variations in the intensity of consciousness can not be adequately measured, it is customary, in studying the relations of the strength of stimulus to the conscious response, to pay attention to the strength of stimu- lus under any given condition which is sufficient to arouse a just perceptible difference in the conscious reaction. Proceeding upon this method, it is found in the case of the visual sensations and the optic nerve, as with other sensations and their corresponding nerves, that the increase of stimulus necessary to cause a just perceptible change in consciousness varies with the amount of stimulus already 318 THE SPECIAL SENSES. acting. If, for instance, the retina is being stimulated by a light of 1 candle power an increase of illumination to 1.1 candle power may make a perceptible difference in sensation. But if the retina is being illuminated b^y a light of 10 candle power an increase to 10.1 candle power would probably make no perceptible difference. For a certain range of stimulation, in fact, it has been stated that the increase in stimulus must be a constant fractional part of the stimu- lus already acting. That is, in the hypothetical case given, if, with 1 candle power, an increase to 1.1 candle power makes a just per- ceptible difference in consciousness, then with 10 candle power an increase of y q- of the acting stimulus, namely — 1 candle power — will be necessary to cause a perceptible difference. The relation as expressed in this form is known as Weber's law ; but it seems prob- able that, while the general fact is true, this exact expression of it holds only approximately for an intermediate range of stimulation. In this matter of a threshold stimulus the sensitiveness of the retina shows also certain interesting differences in the foveal as compared with the peripheral field. The difference is especially marked when the reaction of the retina in strong lights is compared with its reaction in dim lights. The Light-adapted and the Dark-adapted Eye. — The con- dition of the retina changes when after exposure to light it is sub- mitted to darkness, the change being most marked in the peripheral field. The change is known as an adaptation, and in this respect the retina differs from the sensitive photographic plate. When the eye, for instance, is kept in the dark, the sensitiveness in the periph- eral field increases during an hour or so, while that of the foveal field is apparently unchanged. With such a dark-adapted eye, there- fore, there will be a certain dim light which will be seen by the peripheral parts of the retina, but perhaps will cause no reaction upon the fovea. For such a degree of light, therefoi^, the fovea would be blind. This general fact has, indeed, long been known. Anyone may notice in late twilight, when the stars are beginning to appear, that a very faint star may disappear when looked at, — that is, when its image is brought upon the fovea ; to see it one must direct his eyes a little to the side, so as to bring its image into the periph- eral field. This greater sensitiveness of the dark-adapted eye in the peripheral field where the rods predominate over the cones seems to be associated with the movement of the pigment in the pigment epithelium (see above) and the resulting regeneration of the visual purple in the external segments of the rods. The increase in the visual purple in the dark may, indeed, account for the increased sensitiveness to light in the rod-region and explain why a similar increase fails to occur in the fovea, where only cones are present. The curve given in Fig. 136 shows that in the dark-adapted eye PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 319 the acuity of vision in the peripheral field is greater than in the fovea. In accordance with these facts von Kries* has suggested that the rods, the peripheral field of the retina, are especially adapted for vision in dim lights, night vision, while the cones are especially adapted for vision in strong lights, day vision. This general fact will perhaps accord with the experience of anyone who attempts to estimate the value of his peripheral vision in dim nightlight as compared with daylight. Other interesting differences in the reac- tion of the light-adapted and the dark-adapted eye are referred to below in connection with color blindness. CHARACTERISTICS OF THE VISUAL SENSATIONS. In addition to the spatial attributes connected with our visual sensations — that is, the perception of form — they are characterized by two properties which may be described in general as variations in intensity and in quality. Luminosity or Brightness. — That characteristic which we describe as the luminosity or brightness of a visual sensation has been defined differently by various writers. We may consider it, however, as the expression of the intensity of the acting stimulus. Sensations of the same quality are easily compared as regards their brightness. We can tell as between two whites or two greens which is the brighter of the two, but when two different qualities — a red and a green sensation, for instance — are compared our subjective determination of the relative brightness is, for most persons, difficult or impossible to make. To a lesser degree the difficulty is similar to that of the comparison of sight and sound. According to the conception adopted here, however, that the brightness is an ex- pression of the intensity of the stimulus, an objective standard of comparison might be obtained by measuring the resulting action cur- rents in the optic nerve fibers. When the spectral colors are ex- amined it is obvious that some of the colors are brighter than others, the extreme red and extreme violet, for instance, possessing little luminosity as compared with the yellow. The relative brightness of the different spectral colors is found to vary with the amount of ■ illumination, as is shown in the curves given in Fig. 137. With a brilliant spectrum the maximum brightness is in the yellow, but with a feeble illumination it shifts to the green. This fact accords with what is known as the " Purkirije phenomenon," — namely, the changing luminosity and color value of colors in dim lights. As the light becomes more feeble the colors toward the red end of the spectrum lose their quality, the blue colors being perceived last of * Von Kries, "Zeitschrift f. Psychologie u. Physiologie d. Sinnesorgane," 9, 81, 1895. 320 THE SPECIAL SENSES. all. Just as in late twilight it may be noticed that the sky remains distinctly blue after the colors of the landscape become indis- tinguishable. With a very feeble illumination the dark-adapted eye becomes practically totally color blind. Qualities of Visual Sensations. — The different qualities of our color sensations may be arranged in two series: an achromatic series, consisting of white and black and the intermediate grays, and a chromatic series, comprising the various spectral colors, to- gether with the purples made by combination of the two ends of M< u fy M A M i A -V / ( i i \ \ M- / / / / t \ A ifi- / ; *.<, / i V u \ /'/ i ■s \ M *.- . / V. ♦ <,s- ; / ! > \ /,6- ; i i sv \ f.h- ' i i A V I i i '—*••., >.\ A- H , / // \C"V 0,8 ' , / ■ ''/ \ \ Hs s '-.\ 0,6 t ' • \. ox 1 i y^ • / 1 \^> Jr-a-VOM-JW^- ' 610 660 (SU 606 690 iJS 666 Mf SXO 60S k90 +70 ¥SO B C i ) E F 0 Fig. 137. — Diagram showing the distribution of the Intensity of the spectrum as de- pendent upon the degree of illumination. Tin spectrum is represented along the abscissa, the numerals giving the wave lengths from red, 670, to violet, 430. The ordinates give the luminosity of the ilitYerent colors. Bight curves are given to show the changes in distribution of relative brightness with changes in degree of illumination. _ With the greatest illumination the maximum brightness is in the. yellow (605425); with weaker illumination it shifts to the green (535). — (Konia.) the spectrum, red and blue, and the colors obtained by fusion of the spectral colors with white or with black, such, for instance, as the olives and browns. The Achromatic Scries. Our standard white sensation is that caused by sunlight. Objects reflecting to our eye all the visible rays of the sunlight give us a white sensation. This sensation, therefore, is dm4 primarily to the combined action of all the visible rays of the spectrum, each of which, taken separately, would give us a color sensation. White or gray may be produced also by the PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 321 combined action of certain pairs of colors, — complementary colors, — as is described below. Black, on the contrary, is the sensation caused by withdrawal of light. It must be emphasized that in order to see black a retina must be present. It is probable that a person with both eyes enucleated has no sensation of darkness. That black is a sensation referable to a condition of the retina is made probable also by the interesting observations recorded by Gotch,* — namely, that when an eye that has been exposed to light is suddenly cut off from the light there is an electrical change in the retina, a dark response, similar to that caused by throwing light on a retina previously kept in the dark. Blackness, therefore, is a sensation produced by withdrawing light from the retina, and a black object is one that reflects no light to the eye. Black may be combined with white to produce the series of grays, and when com- bined with the spectral colors it gives a series of modified color tones, thus the olives of different shades may be considered as combina- tions of green and black in varying proportions. The chromatic series consists of those qualities to which we give the name of colors, and, as stated above, they comprse the spectral colors, and the extraspectral color, purple, together with the light- weak and light-strong hues obtained by combining the colors with white or black. In the spectrum many different colors may be detected, — some observers record as many as one hundred and sixty, — but in general we give specific names only to those that stand sufficiently far apart to represent quite distinct sensations, — namely, the red, orange, yellow, green, blue, and violet. When light is taken from a definite limited portion of the spectrum we have a monochromatic light that gives us a distinct color sensation varying with the wave length of the portion chosen. Color Saturation and Color Fusion. — The term saturation as applied to colors is meant to define their freedom from accompany- ing white sensation. A perfectly saturated color would be one entirely free from mixture with white. On the objective side it is easy to select* a monochromatic bundle of rays from the spectrum without admixture of white light, but on the physiological side it is not probable that the color sensation thus produced is entirely free from white sensation, since the monochromatic rays may initiate in the retina not only the specific processes underlying the pro- duction of its special color, but at the same time give rise in some degree to the processes causing white sensations. Even the spectral colors are therefore not entirely saturated, but they come as near to giving us this condition as we can get without changing the state of the retina itself by previous stimulation. Color Fvsion. — By color fusion we mean the combination of two * Gotch, "Journal of Physiology," 29, 388, 1903. 21 322 THE SPECIAL SENSES. or more color processes in the retina, this end being obtained by superposing upon the same portion of the retina the rays giving rise to these color processes. It must be borne in mind that color fusion upon the retina is quite a different thing from color mixture as practised by the artist. A blue pigment, such as Prussian blue, for instance, owes its blue color to the fact that when sunlight falls upon it the red-yellow rays are absorbed and only the blue, with some of the green, rays are reflected to the eye. So a yellow pig- ment, chrome yellow, absorbs the blue, violet, and red rays and reflects to the eye only the yellow with some of the green rays. A mixture of the two upon the palette will absorb all the rays except the green and will therefore appear green to the eye. If, however, by means of a suitable device, we throw simultaneously upon the retina a blue and a yellow light, the result of the retinal fusion is a sensation of white. Many different methods have been employed to throw colors simultaneously upon the retina, the most perfect being a system of lenses or mirrors by which different portions of a spectrum can be superposed. The usual device employed in laboratory experiments is that of rotation of discs of colored paper. Each disc has a slit in it from center to periphery so that two discs can be fitted together to expose more or less of each color. If a combination of this kind is attached to a small electrical motor it can be rotated so rapidly that the impressions of the two colors upon the retina follow at such a short interval of time as to be prac- tically simultaneous. The Fundamental and the Complementary Colors. — By the methods of color fusion it can be shown that three colors may be selected from the spectrum whose combinations in different propor- tions will give white or any of the intermediate color shades, or purple. Considered purely objectively, a set of three such colors may be designated as the fundamental colors, and red, yellow, and blue, or red, green, and violet have been the three colors selected. On the physiological side, however, it has been assumed that there are certain more or less independent color processes — photochemical processes — in the retina which give us our fundamental color sen- sations, and that all other color sensations are combinations of these processes in varying proportions with each other or with the proc- esses causing white and black. Referring only to the colors proper, the fundamental color sensations according to some views are red, green, and blue or violet; according to others, they are red, yellow, green, and blue. (See paragraph on Theories of Color Vision.) Helmholtz calls attention to the fact that the names used for these funda- mental color sensations are obviously of ancient origin, thus indicating that the difference in quality of the sensations has been long recognized. Red is from the Sanskrit rudhira, blood; blue from the same root as blow, and re- CO hj "to' If p*3.o ? 2.» o> srs'g.,i Pill II? "3 TO 3 3 ^ w ~>- &*g.l|£|f ' si'i'a 2-2,1 3 i-TO 2 » r* 3 5 3 2 » 3- s » !•§ i s S.« £ <» 5 «- 3 » ^ 3 • •n 0,5 ~~ ~ • ,TO' a*c H.-H £135 • o i e t»<< 3 '*S,-'« ^^ 3-„3P^ »a p a 5.'" ° « > < "* a* 2 2 m s' TO??* 3-0-5^2- 3 2- Iff ft? rr-1 £12 5 O PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 323 fers to the color of the air ; green from the same root as grow, referring to the color of vegetation. Yellow seems to be derived from the same root as gold, which typified the color. The other less distinct qualities have names of recent application, such as orange, violet, indigo blue, etc. Complementary Colors. — It has been found by the methods of color fusion that certain pairs of colors when combined give a white (gray) sensation. It may be said, in fact, that for any given color there exists a complement such that the fusion of the two in suitable proportions gives white. If we confine ourselves to the spectral colors we recognize such complementary pairs as the following: Red and greenish blue. Orange and cyan blue. Yellow and indigo blue. Greenish yellow and violet. The complementary color for green is the extraspectral purple. Colors that are closer together in the spectral series than the complementaries give on fusion some intermediate color which is more saturated — that is, less mixed with white sensation — the nearer the colors are together. ' Thus, red and yellow, when fused, give orange. Colors farther apart than the distance of the comple- mentaries give some shade of purple. On the physical side, there- fore, we can produce a sensation of white in two ways : Either by the combined action of all the visible rays of the spectrum (sunlight) or by the combined action of pairs of colors whose wave lengths vary by a certain interval. It is probable that in the retina the processes induced by these two methods are qualitatively the same, the wave-lengths represented by the complementary colors setting up by their combined action the same photochemical processes that normally are induced by the sunlight. After-images. — As the name implies, this term refers to images that remain in consciousness after the objective stimulus has ceased to act upon the retina. They are due doubtless to the fact that the changes set up in the retina by the visual stimulus continue, with or without modification, after the stimulus is withdrawn. After- images are of two kinds: positive and negative. In the positive after-images the visual sensation retains its normal colors. If one looks at an incandescent electric light for a few seconds and then closes his eyes he continues to see the luminous object for a con- siderable time in its normal colors. Objects of much less intensity of illumination give positive after-images, especially when the eyes have been kept closed for some time, as, for instance, upon waking in the morning. In negative after-images the colors are all reversed, — that is, they take on the complementary qualities (see Fig. 138). White becomes black, red a bluish green, and vice versa. Negative after images are produced very easily by fixing the eyes 324 THE SPECIAL SENSES. steadily upon a given object for an interval of twenty seconds or more and then closing them. In the case of colored objects the after-image is shown better, perhaps, by turning the eyes upon a white surface after the period of fixation is over. After-images produced in this way often appear and disappear a number of times before ceasing entirely, and, although the color at first is the complementary of that of the object looked at, it may change before its final disappearance. Anyone who has gazed for even a brief interval at the setting sun will remember the number of colored and changing after-images seen for a time when the eye is turned to another portion of the sky. That several different after-images are seen in this case is due to the fact that the eyes are not kept fixed under the dazzling light of the sun, and a number of different images are formed, therefore, upon the retina. After-images may be used in a very instructive way to show that our estimates of the size of a retinal image vary with the distance to which we project it, — that is, with the distance at which we suppose we see it. Once the image is, so to speak, branded on the retina, its actual size, of course, does not vary, but our judgment of its size may be made to vary rapidly by projecting the image upon screens at different distances. If, for instance, in obtaining the after-image of the strips shown in Fig. 138 one moves the white paper used to catch the image toward and away from the eye, the apparent size varies proportionally to its distance. Color Contrasts. — By color contrast is meant the influence that one color field has upon a contiguous one. If, for instance, a piece of blue paper is laid upon a larger yellow square, the color of each of them is heightened by contrast. A piece of blue paper on a blue background does not appear so saturated as when placed against a yellow background. The influences of contrast may be shown in a great variety of ways.* For instance, if a disc like that in the illustration, Fig. 140A , is rotated rapidly, it should give circles of gray, the darkest at the middle; but each circle should be uniform as it is made by the fusion of a definite amount of white and black. On the contrary, the appearance obtained is that repre- sented in Fig. 1405. Each circle appears darker on its outer edge where it borders on a lighter circle, and lighter on its inner edge where it borders on a darker circle. Similar contrasts may be obtained from comparing shadows cast by yellow and white light. If a rod be arranged in a dark room so as to cast a shadow from an opening admitting daylight and one also from a lighted candle, either shadow taken separately appears black, but if the two are cast side by side one will appear blue, the other yellow. The shadow cast by the daylight, being illuminated by the yellow candle-light, will appear * Rood, "Modern Chromatics," "International Scientific Series." PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 325 yellow, and the other shadow, that from the candle-light, will by contrast seem quite blue. A striking instance of the effect of con- trast is given, also, by the simple experiment of Mayer, illustrated in Fig. 139. The gray square on the green background suffers no apparent change from contrast, but if the figure is covered by a sheet of white tissue paper the gray square at once takes on a red- dish hue. It is evident that in all artistic and ornamental employ- ment of colors this influence must be considered, and empirical rules are established which indicate for the normal eye the bene- ficial or the killing effect of different colors when brought into juxtaposition. Color Blindness. — The fact that some eyes do not possess normal color vision does not seem to have attracted the attention of scientific observers until it was studied with some care by Dalton, the distinguished English chemist, at the end of the eighteenth century. Dalton himself suffered from color blindness, and the Fig. 1404. — Black and white disc for ex- periment on contrast. — {Rood.) Fig. 140S. — Showing the result when the disc A is set into rapid rotation. — (Rood.) particular variety exhibited by him was for some time described as Daltonism, but is now usually designated as red blindness. The subject was given practical importance by later observers, espe- cially by the Swedish physiologist Holmgren,* who emphasized its relations to possible accidents by rail or at sea in connection with colored signals. It is now the practice in all civilized countries to require tests for color blindness in the case of those who in railways or upon vessels may be responsible for the interpretation of signals. The numerous statistics that have been gathered show that the defect is fairly prevalent, especially among men. It is said that on the average from 2 to 4 per cent, are color blind among males, while among women the proportion is much smaller, — 0.01 to 1 per cent. Among the poorly educated classes the defect is said to be * Holmgren, "Color Blindness in its Relations to Accidents by Rail and Sea," " Smithsonian Institution Reports," Washington, 1878. See also Jeffries, " Color Blindness, its Dangers and its Detection," Boston. 326 THE SPECIAL SENSES. more common than among educated persons. Color blindness may exist in different degrees of completeness, from a total loss to a simple imperfection or feebleness of the color sense, and it is usually congenital. Those who are completely color blind as re- gards some or all of the fundamental colors fall into two groups: the dichromatic, whose color vision may be represented by two fundamental colors and their combinations with white or black, and the monochromatic or totally color blind, who see only the white-gray-black series. Dichromatic Vision. — The color-blind who belong to this class fall into two or three groups, which have been designated, under the influence of the Young-He lmholtz theory of color vision, the red-blind, the green-blind, and the violet-blind. The most com- mon by far of these groups is that of so-called red blindness; it constitutes the usual form of color blindness. As a matter of fact, persons so affected are in reality red-green blind. In what may be called the most typical cases they distinguish in the spectrum only yellows and blues. The red, orange, yellow, and green appear as yellow of different shades, the green- blue as gray, and the blue- violet and purple as blue. The red end of the spectrum is distinctly shortened, especially if the illumination is poor, and the maximum luminosity, instead of being in the yellow as in normal eyes, is in the green. When the spectrum is examined by such persons a neutral gray band is seen at the junction of the blue and green. In some cases, however, this neutral band is not seen, the yellow passing with but little change into the blue. As a matter of fact, in red blindness the most characteristic defect is a failure to see or to appreciate the green. This color is confused with the grays. and with dull shades of red. When such persons are examined for their negative after- images for different colors, it will be noted that they describe some of their after-images as red, the after-image of indigo-blue, for ex- ample, but that they describe none as green. The after-image of purple, for instance, which to the normal eye is bright green, is de- scribed by them as gray blue or pale blue. From the descriptions given it is probable that the color vision of the so-called red blind is not by any means the same in all cases, but exhibits many individ- ual differences. The green-blind are also, according to recent des- criptions, red-green blind; they also confuse reds and greens and in the spectrum are conscious of only two color qualities, namely, yellow and blue. They differ from the red blind in that the red end of the spectrum is not shortened, and the maximum luminosity, as with the normal eye, is placed in the yellow. In the matching and combination of colors they show distinct differences from the red blind, so that though resembling the latter in general features, they differ obviously in some details. Violet blindness, so called, seems PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 327 to be so rare as a congenital and permanent condition that no exact study of it has been made. By the ingestion of santonin it is said that a condition of this kind may be produced temporarily. The violet end of the spectrum is shortened and white objects take on a yellowish hue. The conditions produced by santonin are evidently more complex than can be explained by simply assuming that the violet color sense is lost. Recent observers* state that the drug produces a condition of yellow vision, outside the fovea, in the daylight, and a condition of violet vision with yellow blindness, but no red nor green blindness, in dim lights. Tests for Color Blindness. — Although the vision of the red and the green blind is deficient as regards green and red colors, it will be found in many cases that they recognize these colors and name them correctly, having adopted the usual nomenclature and adapted it to their own standards. In order to detect the deficiency they must be examined by some test which will compel them to match certain colors. Under these circumstances it will be found that along with correct matches they will make others which to the nor- mal eye are entirely erroneous. A great number of methods have been proposed and used to detect color blindness. The simplest perhaps is that of Holmgren. f A number of skeins of wool are used and three test colors are chosen, — namely, (I) a pale pure green skein, which must not incline toward yellow green; (II) a medium purple (magenta) skein; and (III) a vivid red skein. The person under investigation is given skein I and is asked to select from the pile of assorted colored skeins those that have a similar color value. He is not to make an exact match, but to select those that appear to have the same color. Those who are red or green blind will see the test skein as a gray with some yellow or blue shade and will select, therefore, not only the green skeins, but the grays or grayish yellow and blue skeins. To ascertain whether the individual is red or green blind tests II and III may then be employed. With test II, medium purple, the red blind will select, in addition to other purples, only blues or violets ; the green blind will select as "confusion colors" only greens and grays. With test III, red, the red blind will select as confusion colors greens, grays, or browns less luminous than the test color, while the green blind will select greens, grays, or browns of a greater brightness than the test. Monochromatic Vision. — A number of cases of total color blindness have been carefully examined. | It would seem that in such individuals there is an entire loss of color sense, — they possess *Siven and Wendt, "Skandinavisches Archiv f. Physiologie," 14, 196, 1903. t For details see the works of Holmgren and of Jeffries, already quoted. t Grunert, "Archiv fur Ophthalmologic," 56, 132, 1903. 328 THE SPECIAL SENSES. only achromatic vision. The external world appears to them only in shades of gray. In the majority of these cases (f) there is a region of blindness in the fovea (central scotoma), and an unusual sensitiveness to light and nystagmus (rolling movement of the eyeballs) are also characteristic. Since the peripheral field of vision is nearly normal as regards sensitiveness to light, while the central field is frequently blind or amblyopic, it has been assumed that this condition represents one of loss of function in the cones. Distribution of the Color Sense in the Retina. — What has been said above in regard to color blindness refers especially to the °Sl 081 oU Fig. 141. — Perimeter chart indicating the average fields of vision for blue, red, and green compared with white (gray). Right eye: The outlines of the color fields are repre- sented as smooth since the chart is an average from many determinations. As a matter of fact, in each individual the outline is highly irregular. Normally green (bright green) is the smallest field, green objects outside the limit appearing yellow and farther out colorless (gray). central field of vision. When we examine the peripheral field in the normal eye it is found that on the extreme periphery the retina is totally color blind, perceiving only light and darkness, — that is, the shades of gray. As we pass in toward the center the color sense develops gradually, the blue colors being perceived first and the greens last, — that is, nearest to the center, — so that in a cer- tain zone the normal eye is red-green blind. The distribution of the color sense may be studied conveniently by means of the pe- rimeter (see p. 313). It will be found to vary with each individual, so much so that it is possible that a test of this character might be PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 329 used for the identification of individuals. Exceptionally it is found that the entire retina possesses a nearly normal color sense. Usu- ally, for the colors red, green, and blue, the blue has the most exten- sive field and the green the least, as is indicated in the perimeter chart given in Fig. 141. If the green chosen is blue green (490/*/*) — that is, the complementary of the red — it is stated that their fields are co-extensive.* From this standpoint the retina presents three concentric zones: an extreme peripheral zone devoid of color vision, an intermediate zone in which yellow and blue are perceived, and a central zone sensitive to red and green. The outlines of *' ON 'W OH u)V Fig. 142. — Perimeter chart showing the highly restricted color fields in the left eye of a typical case of so-called red-green color blindness. The ability to distinguish red and green, by whatever characteristics of intensity or color they possessed extended for a very short distance outside the fovea. It is interesting that the ability to distinguish blue was in this case limited as compared with a normal eye. the different fields usually show many irregularities, and in some cases it will be found that bright green is perceived over a larger area than the red. The fields are not identical in the two eyes, and in each eye it is, as a rule, more extensive upon the nasal than upon the temporal side of the retina. In the red-green blind the peripheral fields of color vision, judged by the individual's own standards, may be markedly constricted as compared with the nor- mal retina (see Fig. 142). * Baird, "The Color Sensitivity of the Peripheral Retina," Carnegie Publication No. 29, 1905. 330 THE SPECIAL SENSES. Functions of the Rods and Cones. — Many facts unite in mak- ing it probable that the rods and cones are different in function. They differ in structure and especially in their connections. As is shown in the diagram given in Fig. 143, the cones terminate in the external nuclear layer in arborizations which connect with the Fig. 143.— Schema of the structure of the human retina (Greeff) : I, Pigment layer; II, rod and cone layer; ///, outer nuclear layer; IV, external plexiform layer; V, layer of horizontal cells; VI, layer of bipolar cells (inner nuclear); VII, layer of amacrinal cells (without axons); VIII, inner plexiform layer; IX, ganglion cell layer; X, nerve fiber layer; 6, fiber of Miiller. bipolar ganglion cells, and in the fovea at least this connection is such that each cone connects with a single nerve cell and eventually per- haps with a single optic nerve fiber. The rods, on the contrary, end in a single knob-like swelling, and a number of them make con- nections with the same nerve cell. Histologically, therefore, the PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 331 conduction paths for the cones seem to be more direct than in the case of the rods. These latter elements, moreover, possess the visual purple, which is lacking in the cones. Lastly, in the eye of the totally color blind, in the dark-adapted eye in dim lights, in the color-blind peripheral area of the normal eye, and in the eyes of most distinctly night-seeing animals, such as the mole and the owl, vision seems to be effected solely by the rods. These facts find their simplest explanation perhaps in the view advocated by Pari- naud, Franklin, von Kries,* and others, according to which the perception of color is a function of the cones alone, while the rods are sensitive only to light and darkness, and by virtue of their power of adaptation in the dark through the regeneration of their visual purple they form also the special apparatus for vision in dim lights (night vision). Color blindness, therefore, whether total or partial, may be regarded as an affection or lack of normal develop- ment of the cones. On the other hand, those interesting cases in which the vision, while good in daylight, is faulty or lacking in dim lights (night blindness, hemeralopia) may be referred to a defective functional activity of the rods, probably from lack of formation of visual purple. Theories of Color Vision. — A number of theories have been proposed to explain the facts of color vision. None of them has been entirely successful in the sense that the explanations it affords have been submitted to satisfactory experimental verification. The immediate stimuli that give rise to the visual impulses are assumed to be of a chemical nature, and it seems probable that in this case as in that of many other problems of physiology, we must await the development of a more complete knowledge of the chemical processes involved. The theories proposed at present, while all tested by experimental inquiries, are in a large measure hypotheses constructed to fit more or less completely the facts that are known. Three of these theories may be described briefly as examples of the modes of reasoning employed: /. The Young-Helmholtz Theory. — This theory, proposed essen- tially by Thomas Young (1807) and afterward modified and ex- panded by Helmholtz,t rests upon the assumption that there are three fundamental color sensations, — red, green, and violet — and corresponding with these there are three photochemical substances in the retina. By the decomposition of each of these substances cor- responding nerve fibers are stimulated and impulses are conducted to a special system of nerve cells in the visual center of the cerebrum. *Von Kries, "Zeitschrift f. Psychologie u. Physiol, d. Sinnesorgane," 9, 81, 1895. f Helmholtz, "Handbuch der physiologischen Optik," second edition, 1896, 1, 344. 332 THE SPECIAL SENSES. The theory, therefore, assumes special nerve fibers and nerve centers corresponding respectively to the red, green, and violet photo- chemical substances, and the peculiar quality of the resulting sensa- tions are referred, in the original theory, to the different reactions in consciousness in the three corresponding centers in the brain. When these three substances are equally excited a sensation of white results, of greater or less intensity according to the extent of the excitation. White, therefore, on this theory, is a compound sensation produced by the combination or fusion in consciousness of the three equal fundamental color sensations. The sensation of black, on the other hand, results from the absence of stimulation, from the condition of rest in the retina and in the corresponding nerve fibers and nerve centers. All other color sensations — yellow, for instance — are compound sensations produced by the combined Fig. 144. — Schema to illustrate the Young-Helmholtz theory of color vision. — (Helm- holtz.) The spectral colors are arranged in their natural order, — red to violet. The curves represent the intensity of stimulation of the three color substances: 1, The red perceiving substance; 2, the green perceiving; 3, the violet perceiving. Verticals drawn at any point of the spectrum indicate the relative amount of stimulation of the three substances for that wave length of the spectrum. stimulation of the three photochemical substances in different propor- tions. It is assumed, furthermore, that each of the photochemical substances is acted upon more or less by all of the visible rays of the spectrum, but that the rays of long wave lengths at the red end of the spectrum affect chiefly the red substance, those corresponding to the green of the spectrum chiefly the green substance, and the rays of shortest wave length chiefly the violet substance. These rela- tionships are expressed in the diagram given in Fig. 144. The figure also indicates that it is impossible to stimulate any one of these sub- stances entirely alone, — that is, we cannot obtain a perfectly satu- rated color sensation. Even the extreme red or the extreme violet rays act more or less on all of the substances, and the resulting red or violet sensation, is, therefore, mixed to some extent with white, — that is, is not entirely saturated. The theory, as stated by Helm- holtz, held strictly to the doctrine of specific nerve energy, in assuming PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 333 that each photochemical substance serves simply as a means for the excitation of a nerve fiber, and that the quality of the sensation aroused depends on the ending of this fiber in the brain. The phe- nomenon of negative after-images finds a simple explanation in terms of this theory. If we look fixedly at a green object, for example, the corresponding photochemical substance is chiefly acted upon, and if subsequently the same part of the retina is exposed to white light, the red and violet substances, having been previously less acted upon, now respond in greater proportions to the white light, and the after-image takes a red- violet — that is, purple — color. Many objections have been raised to the Young- Helmholtz theory. It has been urged, for instance, that we are not conscious that white or yellow sensations are blends or compounded color sensations; we perceive in them none of the supposed component elements as we do in such undoubted mixtures as the blue-greens or the purples. The theory explains poorly or not at all the fact that on the periphery of the retina we are color blind and yet can perceive white or gray, and it breaks down also in the face of the facts of partial and com- plete color blindness. The explanation given for black is also unsatisfactory in that it assumes an active state of consciousness associated with a condition of rest in the visual mechanism. 77. Hering's Theory of Color Vision. — This theory also assumes the existence in the retina of three photochemical substances, but of such a nature as to give us six different qualities of sensation. There is a white-black substance which when acted upon by the visible rays of light undergoes disassimilation and sets up nerve impulses that arouse in the brain the sensation of white. On the other hand, when not acted upon by light this same substance under- goes assimilatory processes that in turn set up nerve impulses which in the brain give us a sensation of black. There are in the retina also a red-green and a yellow-blue substance. The former when acted upon by the longer rays undergoes disassimilation and gives a sensation of red, while the shorter waves cause assimilation and produce a sensation of green. A similar assumption is made for the yellow-blue substance. The essence of the theory may be stated, therefore, in tabular form, as follows *: Photochemical Substance. Retinal Process. Sensation. P _. f Disassimilation = red Keel-green - — : — .:=- Fig. 162. — Diagrammatic view of the organ of Corti, the sense cells, and the accessory structures of the membranous cochlea (Testut): A, Inner rods of Corti ; B, outer rods of Corti ; C, tunnel of Corti ; D, basilar membrane; E, single row of inner hair (sense) cells; 6, 6', 6", rows of outer hair (sense) cells; 7, 7', supporting cells of Deiters. The ends of the inner hair cells are seen projecting through the openings of the reticulate membrane. The terminal arborizations of the cochlear nerve fibers end around the inner and outer hair cells. description must be given of the nature of sound waves and the physical facts in regard to sympathetic resonance. The Nature and Action of Sound Waves. — Sound waves in air consist of longitudinal vibrations of the air molecules, alternate phases of rarefaction and condensation. For convenience' sake, these waves are usually represented graphically after the manner of water waves, by a curved line rising above and falling below a median zero line, the ordinates above the zero line representing the phase of condensation, and those below the phase of rarefaction. These waves are produced by the vibrations of the sounding body, and may vary greatly in length, in amplitude, and in form. For 362 THE SPECIAL SENSES. musical sounds within the range of hearing the length of the waves may vary from forty to seventy feet, at the one extreme, to a frac- tion of an inch at the other. They travel through the air with an average velocity of 1100 to 1200 feet per second, the exact rate vary- ing with the temperature. When these waves, whatever may be their form, follow each other with regularity — that is, with a definite period or rhythm — a musical sound is perceived provided the rhythm is maintained for a number of vibrations. So that regular- ity or periodicity of the sound waves may be considered as the un- derlying physical cause of musical sounds. Non-musical sounds or noises, which constitute the vast majority of our auditory sensa- tions, are referred, on the contrary, to non-periodical vibrations. Waves of this kind may be due to the nature of the impulse given to the air by the sounding body, — single pulses, for instance, or a series of such pulses or shocks following at a slow or irregular rhythm, or as is more frequently the case, they may result from a mixture of very short and different rhythmical vibrations. As the case of musical sounds is far the simpler, the theory of the action of the cochlea has been based chiefly upon the results obtained from a study of these forms of waves. Classification and Properties of Musical Sounds. — Musical sounds exhibit three fundamental properties, each of which may be referred to a difference in the physical stimulus. They vary, in the first place, in pitch, and this difference finds its explanation in the rapidity of vibration of the sounding body and the sound waves produced by it. The more rapid the rate, the shorter will be the waves and the higher will be the pitch of the musical note. Notes of the same pitch may, however, vary in loudness or intensity, and this difference is referable to the amplitude of the vibrations. A given tuning fork emits always a note of the same pitch, but the loudness of the note may vary according to the amplitude of the vibrations. The vibrations of the tympanic membrane and of the perilymph in the internal ear vary in rate and intensity with the sounding body; so that we may say that the stimulation of the hair cells in the cochlea gives us auditory sensations that vary in pitch with the rate of excitation and in intensity with the ampli- tude of the vibratory movement. A third property of musical sounds is their variations in quality or timbre. The same note of the same amplitude when given by different musical instruments varies in quality, so that we have no difficulty in recognizing the note of a piano from the same note when given by a violin or the human voice. The underlying physical cause of variations in timbre is found in the form of the sound waves produced, and immediately, therefore, in the form of vibratory movement communicated to the perilymph. Examination of the forms of sound waves produced EAR AS AN ORGAN FOR SOUND SENSATIONS. 363 by different musical instruments shows that they may be divided into two great groups: (1) The simple or pendular form; (2) the compound or non-pendular form. The simple or pendular form of wave is given, for instance, by tuning forks. A graphic repre- sentation of this wave form may be obtained by attaching a bristle to the end of the fork and allowing it to write upon a piece of black- ened paper moving with uniform velocity, — the blackened surface, for instance, of a kymographion. The form of the wave obtained is represented in Fig. 163. The vibrating body swings symmetrically to each side of the line of rest, and, inasmuch as this is also the form of movement that would be traced by a swinging pendulum, this form of wave is designated frequently as pendular. It is sometimes called also the sinusoidal wave, since the distance of the vibrating point to each side of the line of rest is equal to the sine of an arc increasing proportionally for the time of the phase. A compound (or non-pendular or non-sinusoidal) wave may have a very great variety of forms. The different phases follow periodically, but the movement of the vibrating body to each side of the line of rest is not Fig. 163. — Form of wave made by tuning fork. perfectly symmetrical. Fourier has shown that any periodical vibra- tory movement, whatever may be its form, may be considered as being composed of a series of simple or pendular movements whose periods of vibrations are 1, 2, 3, 4, etc., times as great as the vibra- tion period of the given movement. That is, every so-called com- pound wave form may be considered as being caused by the fusion of a number of simple waves. Representing the wave movement of the air graphically as water waves, this composition of simple waves into compound ones is illustrated by the curves given in Fig. 164. In this figure A and B represent two simple vibrations such as would be given by two tuning-forks, the vibrations in B being double those of A. If these two waves are communicated to the air at the same time the actual movement of the molecules will be a resultant of the forces acting upon them at any given instant, and the actual movement will be indicated, therefore, by the algebraical sum of the ordinates above and below the lines of rest. If the movements are so timed that e in curve B is synchronous with d° in curve A , then the resulting compound wave form is illustrated by C. If, however, curve B is supposed to be in a different phase, so that e is synchronous with df, then a form of wave illustrated by D will be 364 THE SPECIAL SENSES. obtained. In this way a great variety of forms of compound waves may be supposed to be produced by the union of a series of simple waves of different periods of vibration. That compound waves dif- fer from simple ones in being composed of several series of vibrations is indicated directly by our sensations. When we listen to the note of a tuning-fork we hear only a single tone; when two or more tuning-forks are sounded together the trained ear can detect the tone due to each fork, and similarly when a single note is sounded by the human voice, a violin, or any other instrument that has a char- acteristic quality the trained ear can detect a series of higher tones, Fig. 164. — Schema by Helmholtz to illustrate the formation of a compound wave from two pendular waves: A and B, pendular vibrations, B being the octave of A. If superposed so that e coincides with d° and the ordinates are added algebraically, the non- pendular curve C is produced. If superposed so that e coincides with d' the non-pendular curve D is produced. the upper partial tones or harmonics or overtones, which indicate that the note is really compound, and not simple. The formation of these overtones is due to the fact that the sounding body vibrates not only as a whole, but also in its aliquot parts, as may be repre- sented in Fig. 165, illustrating the vibrations of a string. When the string is plucked it vibrates as a whole (a), giving large waves which produce what is called the fundamental tone, but at the same time each half (6), third (c), fourth (d), etc., may vibrate, giving each its own simple tone. The combination of all of these simple waves forms a compound wave whose form or at least whose EAR AS AN ORGAN FOR SOUND SENSATIONS. 365 composition determines the quality of the tone heard. As many as ten or sixteen of these overtones may be detected from the vibrating strings of a violin or guitar. When the period of vibra- tion of these overtones bears a simple ratio to that of the funda- mental, a ratio that can be expressed by the simple numbers, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, they harmonize with it and form the harmonic overtones. It should be borne in mind that, so far as the tympanic membrane is concerned, it does not respond separately to the single tones which constitute the compound wave, but swings in unison with the movement of the compound wave. Nevertheless the internal cO ~b^- 0 ^-^ Fig. 165 — To illustrate the mechanism of the formation of overtones. — (Helmholtz.) In a the string vibrates as a whole, giving its fundamental tone; in b, c, and d, its halves, thirds, and fourths are vibrating independently. When a string is struck, plucked, or bowed these movements may happen simultaneously and the fundamental note due to the vibra- tions of the whole string is combined with the notes due to the vibrations of aliquot parts, the overtones. The combination gives a compound wave whose form and musical quality vary with the number and relative strength of the overtones. ear, according to the law of Ohm, is capable of analyzing the com- pound wave form into the series of simple or pendular wrave forms of which it is composed and of distinguishing the series of corre- sponding tones. While this analysis cannot be made consciously except by the trained musician, it is made unconsciously, as it were, by every normal ear, and in consequence of this analysis we recognize the variations in quality of different compound tones. The principle upon which the cochlea acts in thus separating the compound tones into their elements is not explained with entire satisfaction. According to the view so admirably presented by 366 THE SPECIAL SENSES. Helmholtz,* the analysis depends upon the existence in the ear of a mechanism for sympathetic vibrations or resonance. Sympathetic Vibrations or Resonance. — By sympathetic vibration is meant the fact that an elastic body is easily set into vibration by movements of the surrounding medium when these movements correspond with its own period of vibration. A string whose period of vibration is 128 per second will be little affected by vibrations of the surrounding air unless they have the same periodicity. If, however, a note of this period is sounded by the voice, for instance, the string will be set into vibration with rela- tive ease. By means of this principle the untrained ear can readily pick out the more prominent of the upper harmonics of any given note of a musical instrument. It is only necessary to select a series of resonators corresponding to the series of overtones. Each reso- nator is set into vibration by its corresponding overtone and so emphasizes this particular tone that it may be easily recognized. If one stands in front of a piano with the strings exposed and sings a given note it may be shown that a series of the piano strings is set into vibration corresponding, in the first place, to the rate of vibration of the fundamental tone, and secondly to the more prominent of the harmonic overtones. In this case the com- pound wave strikes upon the collection of strings of the piano, and is analyzed into its component simple tones by the sympa- thetic vibrations of the corresponding strings. Helmholtz assumes that the cochlea analyzes compound musical waves by an essentially similar method. The Functions of the Cochlea. — The vibratory movement, whatever may be its form, in the air of the external meatus im- parts to the tympanic membrane a similar form of movement, and this, in turn, through the ear bones and the membrane of the fenes- tra ovalis sets the perilymph into vibrations of the same form. That the perilymph can swing or vibrate under the influence of the move- ments of the stapes is explained by the existence of the second opening, the fenestra rotunda, between the middle and the inter- nal ear (see Fig. 157). As the membrane of the fenestra ovalis is pushed in, that of the fenestra rotunda is pushed out, and vice versa. These vibratory movements of the perilymph affect the membranous cochlea, which may be regarded as being suspended in the perilymph, and according to the resonance theory certain structures within the membranous cochlea are set into sympa- thetic vibrations corresponding to the simple waves of which the compound wave is constituted. Helmholtz first suggested that the peculiar rods of Corti form the resonating apparatus and by sympathetic vibrations are capable of analyzing the compound * Helmholtz, loc. cit. EAR AS AN ORGAN FOR SOUND SENSATIONS. 367 movement. Later, however, this suggestion was abandoned, since the number of the rods is not sufficiently great perhaps to answer the requirements of this theory. According to Retzius, the inner rods number 5600 and the outer ones 3850. Moreover, these structures are absent from the bird's cochlea, and we must assume that these animals are capable of appreciating musical sounds. Helmholtz then adopted a suggestion of Hensen's, that the basilar membrane constitutes the resonating apparatus. This membrane forms the floor of the membranous cochlea, stretching from the limbus to the opposite side of the bony cochlea (Fig. 162). Its middle layer consists of fibers, running radially, which, though united to one another, are sufficiently independent to be regarded as separate strings. These fibers in the portion covered by the rods of Corti, the inner zone or zona tecta, are finer and more difficult to separate than in the portion exterior to the outer rods, the outer zone or zona pectinata. From the base to the apex of the cochlea the membrane increases in width, the length of the strings in the outer zone varying, according to Retzius, from 135 fJ. in the basal portion to 220 ft in the middle spiral and to 234 /j. at the apex. The whole structure is estimated to contain about 24,000 strings varying gradually in length, as stated, and resembling in general arrangement the strings of the piano. Assuming that each of these fibers has its own period of vibration, we may imagine that the entire collection forms an apparatus for sympathetic vibration which is capable of analyzing each compound wave motion into its constituent simple waves, each string being set into strong- est vibrations by the wave of the corresponding period. More- over, it is implied or assumed in this theory that the vibrations of each string are communicated to a corresponding nerve fiber of the cochlear nerve, through which the stimulus is conveyed to the brain as a nerve impulse. We should be capable of perceiv- ing, theoretically, as many distinct musical tones as there are fibers in the basilar membrane, while a compound wave, by setting a number of these mechanisms into action, gives a series of sensations which are more or less fused in consciousness. The peculiar quality or timbre of the tone of each instrument is refer- able, therefore, immediately to the number and relative intensities of the simple tone sensations that it arouses. The fusion of these elementary tone sensations into compound ones of different qual- ities is comparable, in a general way, to the fusion of simple color sensations, with this exception, however, that in the compound tone sensations we are capable of distinguishing more clearly the fact that they are composed of simpler elements ; the constituent tones may be recognized by the trained ear at least. The mechan- ism by which the vibrations of the strings of the basilar mem- 368 THE SPECIAL SENSES. brane are conveyed to the hair cells and through them to the nerve fibers is a matter of speculation only, as are also the functions of the remaining parts of the organ of Corti. It may be suggested, perhaps, that the rods of Corti and Deiters's cells, together with the reticulate membrane, with which they are both connected, form not only a supporting apparatus for the hair cells, but also a mechanism by which the vibrations of the strings are commu- nicated to the hairs of the hair cells; but the suggestion is unsatis- factory, as the anatomical arrangement does not suffice to explain how the vibrations of individual strings are transmitted to the separate hair cells. The assumption has also been made that the tectorial membrane acts as a damper to the vibrating hair cells or the reticulate membrane. Its position as a pad lying over the rods of Corti and the reticulate membrane justifies perhaps such an assumption. Many physiologists, while accepting the general principle that the cochlea analyzes the sound waves by a mechan- ism for sympathetic vibrations, have been unwilling to admit that the basilar membrane constitutes such a mechanism. They point to the improbability or impossibility of fibers of only 0.36 mm. (or 0.5 mm. at the best) in length acting as efficient reso- nators, especially as they are not entirely free and are surrounded by liquid. Attempts have been made, therefore, to select other structures in the cochlea as more likely to be affected by sympa- thetic vibrations. Attention has been directed mainly to the tectorial membrane or membrane of Corti. Thus, Ayers* believes that this structure as seen in the usual microscopical prepara- tions, is simply an artefact. Under normal conditions he believes that it is a band of very long and delicate hairs projecting from the hair cells and lying free in the endolymph. According to his view, it is these hairs that take up the vibrations and transmit their impulses directly to the hair cells. The histological statement upon which this view is based has not, however, been verified. More recently v. Ebner,f reviving an older view of Hasse, has suggested that the tectorial membrane, especially its free end, serves as the mechanism for sympathetic vibration. This membrane increases in width from the base to the apex of the cochlea and varies in thickness in its radial diameter, so that it might be conceived to respond to different periods of vibrations in its different parts, its movements being communicated directly to the hair cells upon which it rests. Unfortunately we have no direct experimental evidence in favor of any of these views. Several observers, how- ever, have demonstrated apparently that, whatever may be the * Ayers, "Journal of Morphology," 6, 1, 1892. t Kolliker, " Handbuch d. Gewebelehre," sixth edition, vol. iii, pt. n, p. 958, 1902. EAR AS AN ORGAN FOR SOUND SENSATIONS. 369 mechanism for sympathetic vibration, it is so arranged that at the base of the cochlea the higher notes are received and at the apex the notes of the lowest pitch. Thus, Munk, in experiments upon dogs, in which by an operation through the fenestra rotunda he had destroyed the basal portion of the cochlea, found that the animals, after a temporary deafness of some days, could hear apparently only low tones and noises. Baginsky,* in a later series of experi- ments, opened the bulla ossea on each side, destroyed the cochlea on one side entirely so as to render that ear deaf, while on the other he injured it in certain areas only. He found that when the apex of the cochlea was des- troyed the animal appeared to perceive only the high tones, c'" ', c"", c'"". The fundamental principle of the theory of the function of the cochlea as developed by Helmholtz has been subjected to some criticism. The theory of a series of resonators each responding to a definite note does not explain with entire satisfaction some of the known acoustic phenomena. Thus, it is known that when two notes are sounded together combinational tones may be heard, either a low difference tone whose pitch is equal to that of the difference between the rates of the two notes, or a summation tone whose pitch is equal to the sum of the vibrations of the two notes. It is difficult to conceive that these combina- tional tones have an objective existence, as vibrations, and the means by which they are perceived by the cochlea is not explained satisfactorily by the theory of resonators. Other theories of the function of the cochlea have been proposed to avoid such difficulties. Thus, Ewald f suggests a view according to which the basilar membrane vibrates throughout its length for each note. He has shown that a rubber membrane of the dimensions of the basilar membrane will be set into such vibrations throughout its length and when examined under the microscope presents such a pic- ture as is represented in Fig. 166, in which the crests of the waves are at a fixed interval for each tone. If at these intervals the corresponding hair cells and nerve fibers are supposed to be stimulated, then our consciousness would recognize each note by its appro- priate interval. For the application of this theory to combinational tones and beats — reference must be made Fig. 166.— To il- lustrate the idea of a fixed sound wave. — {Ewald.) The illus- tration shows a fun- damental note and its first overtone. musical harmony — to the original. Sensations of Harmony and Discord. — The combination of notes to produce various harmonies or intentional discords is a part of the theory of music, but attention may be called briefly to the physiological explanation offered by Helmholtz to account for the fact that certain notes when combined give us a disagreeable sen- sation, appear rough, and unpleasant; while others, on the contrary, produce pleasant sensations. Discord or dissonance is due, accord- * Baginsky, "Virchow's Archiv f. pathol. Anat.," 94, 65, 1883. t Ewald, "Archiv f. d. gesammte Physiologie," 76, 147, 1899. 24 370 THE SPECIAL SENSES. ing to Helmholtz, to the beats produced when two dissonant notes are sounded together. On the physical side the beat, — that is, a rhythmical variation in the intensity of the sound, — is due to the phenomenon of interference. If the rates of vibration of two notes are such that at certain intervals the crests of the waves fall to- gether and again the crest of one coincides with the hollow of the other, the sound sensations will be periodically increased and decreased. While there is no fundamental explanation for the fact that a regularly varying intensity of sound is disagreeable, it is a well-known phenomenon and it finds analogies in the other sensations, — for instance, in the very disagreeable effect of a flick- ering light. When two notes are sounded together the number of beats varies with the difference between the rates of vibration; thus, two notes, one of 128 vibrations and the other of 136 vibra- tions, give 8 beats per second. When the number of beats rises to 33 per second the discord is most disagreeable ; if, however, the rate of interference is more rapid, the unpleasant sensation becomes less perceptible, and beyond 132 per second is not notice- able. When the rates of vibrations of two tones are such that neither the fundamentals nor any of the overtones give beats, the effect is that of harmony, the vibrations of one note strengthening that of the other. The most perfect harmony is that of a note sounded simultaneously with another of the same rate, ratio 1:1, or with its octave, ratio 1: 2. The various intervals which in music have been found to be perfectly consonant or which vary so little from it as to be usable in harmonies are those whose vibra- tions bear a simple ratio to each other. Thus, the octave of any note has the ratio of 1:2, the double octave 1 : 4, the twelfth 1 : 3. These three intervals give absolutely consonant sounds. Other intervals — such as the fifth, 2:3, or the major third, 4: 5 — give a less perfect consonance. Three or more notes bearing such rela- tions to each other constitute a chord, the vibrations in the major chord being, for instance, in the ratios 4:5:6, — c' (128), e' (160), g' (192). The Limits of Hearing. — The rates of vibration that can be perceived by the ear as musical tones lie between fairly well- defined limits, although in this organ, as in the case of the eye, there are individual variations, — variations, indeed, which are more marked in the case of the ear, since its range of appreciation is larger. The lowest rate of vibration that can cause a musical sensation is usually placed at 28 to 30 per second, although some ears can still respond to an octave lower, about 16 per second. To most ears vibrations below 30 per second are felt, if perceived at all, as single pulses that stimulate the sensory nerves of the tympanic membrane itself. The cochlea does not respond. It may happen, EAR AS AN ORGAN FOR SOUND SENSATIONS. 371 however, that a vibration too slow to be perceived by the ear will give overtones of sufficient strength to be recognized. An inter- esting example in physiology of this fact is furnished by the tone of the contracting muscle. As heard, this tone corresponds to a vibration of 40 per second; but other data lead us to believe that the vibrations of the contracting muscle, due to the single con- tractions of which the compound contraction is composed, occur at the rate of only 10 per second; so that what is heard is, in reality, the second octave of the fundamental. The high limit of audibility, on the other hand, is usually placed at 40,000 double vibrations per second, although the various estimates published vary so widely that in this respect there must be great individual differences. The shrill notes of insects are said to be inaudible to some ears. Konig, making use of Kundt's method of light powders, succeeded in tuning a series of forks to an estimated rate of 90,000 double vibrations per second. It was found that those between c7 and c9 (8192 to 32,768) were generally audible, while the c10 (65,536) was inaudible. The limit, therefore, lay between c9 and c10. Notes near this high limit are not, however, usable in ordinary music; the sensations produced have a disagreeable, if not actually painful shrillness. The range of vibrations employed in music is illustrated by the seven octaves of the piano, the notes varying from the lowest c of 32 vibrations to c6 of 4096 vibrations. The intervening series is divided into tones whose serial relations to each other are ex- pressed by the ratios f or V° and semitones of the ratio Tf or -ff ; thus, c" = 256 vibrations and the d" of the same octave corresponds to 256 X f = 288 vibrations* :'See Helmholtz, Popular Scientific Lectures, "Ueberdie physiologischen Ursachen des musikalischen Harmonie," Bonn, 1857. CHAPTER XXI. THE FUNCTIONS OF THE SEMICIRCULAR CANALS AND THE VESTIBULE. Position and Structure. — The membranous semicircular canals lie within the bony semicircular canals, the space between being filled with perilymph which communicates freely with that in the rest of the labyrinth. Within the membranous canals is the endo- lymph, which communicates through the five openings with the endolymph in the utriculus. The canals lie in three planes that are, approximately at least, at right angles to each other (Fig. 167). The horizontal canals lie in a horizontal plane at right angles to the mesial or sagittal plane of the body, the vertical canals on each side make an angle of about 45 degrees with this mesial plane. The plane of each of the anterior canals is parallel to that of the posterior or inferior vertical canal of the opposite side, as represented in the figure. At one end of each canal near its junction with the utriculus is the swelling known as the ampulla and witnin the ampulla lies the crista acustica containing the hair cells with which the nerve fibers communicate, and which, therefore, are considered as the sense cells of the organ. The hair cells are cylin- drical and each gives off a long hair, consisting perhaps of a bundle of finer hairs, which projects into the interior of the canal for a distance of at least 28 fi. The nerve fibers distributed to these hair cells are given off by the vestibular branch of the eighth nerve, or more properly the vestibular nerve, one branch of which (ramus utriculo-ampullaris) supplies the utriculus and the ampulla of the superior and horizontal canals, while the other (ramus sacculo-ampullaris) furnishes fibers to the sacculus and the posterior ampulla. Flourens's Experiments upon the Semicircular Canals. — Modern experiments and theories concerning the functions of the semicircular canals date from the classical researches of Flourens* * Flourens, "Recherches experimentales sur les proprieties et les fonc- tions du systeme nerveux," second edition, 1842. 372 Fig. 167. -Diagrammatic hori- zontal section through the head to il- lustrate the planes occupied by the semicircular canals (after Waller) : S, Superior canal; P, posterior canal; H, horizontal canal. SEMICIRCULAR CANALS AND THE VESTIBULE. 373 (1824). This investigator laid bare the canals in birds and mam- mals and studied the effects of sections of one or more of them. The experiments have since been repeated by numerous observers, and the results obtained have been described in great detail, for an account of which reference must be made to original sources.* In general, it may be said that injuries to the canals are followed by certain more or less definite movements of the head, eyes, and body, and by a disturbance in the power of the animal to co-ordinate nor- mally the muscles used in standing, locomotion, or flying. The character and extent of these results vary with the number of canals injured, and, indeed, show a more or less definite relationship to the several canals. When the horizontal canal is cut on one side in pigeons the animal makes movements of the head in the plane of that canal, and if the similar canal on the other side is also sec- tioned these movements are more pronounced. The animal may also in moving show an inability to walk normally and a tendency, especially when excited, to make abnormal forced movements of rotation of the whole body. After such an operation the pigeon will not fly voluntarily and if thrown into the air is not able to guide its flight with accuracy and soon descends. Similar operations on the anterior or the posterior canals cause movements of the head in the corresponding planes and a tendency in walking or flying to make forced movements — somersaults — forward or backward. When all three canals are cut on one or both sides the animal shows a distressing inability to maintain a normal position. The head is twisted, it is not able to stand unless supported, and any attempt at walking or flying results in violent forced and inco-ordinated movements. The animal makes continual somersaults at each attempt to stand or walk and the head is kept in spasmodic, forceful movements, which may produce injury or death. To preserve the animal from injury after such an extensive operation it is necessary to keep it wrapped in bandages. It should be added that results of this character are obtained only when the membranous canals are injured. If the bony canal alone is cut and even if the peri- lymph is removed by suction no such effects are obtained. At most slight and relatively transient movements of the head are observed. If the exposed membranous canal is pricked with a needle more violent movements result, and if sectioned these move- ments are maintained for a longer period and are accompanied by the other results described. Similar effects have been obtained from operations on mammals and other animals, but the results * The literature of the semicircular canals and the vestibule is very ex- tensive. The complete bibliography may be obtained from the following sources: "Die Lehren von den Funktionen der einzelnen Theile des Ohrlaby- rinths," by von Stein, 1894; Richet's " Dictionnaire de Physiologie," article by Cyon, on "Espace," 1900. 374 THE SPECIAL SENSES. are more pronounced in some animals than in others, varying apparently with the delicacy of the co-ordination necessary to the movements (Ewald). Thus, the movements of walking or flying in the pigeon may be assumed to require a nicer adjustment of the muscles used than is necessary in the swimming movements of the fish, and in correspondence with this idea it is found that opera- tions on the canals of fishes are not followed by conspicuous effects upon the movements of the animals. Temporary and Permanent Effects of the Operation. — The general effects of operations on the semicircular canals, so far as disturbances of equilibrium and occurrence of forced movements are concerned, resemble those resulting from operations upon the cerebellum, and, as in the case of the last mentioned organ, it is found by most observers that if the animal is properly cared for the severity of the first effects passes off to a greater or less extent. Flourens states that his pigeons, with two or more canals cut, continued to show the effects of the operation almost with the same intensity for nearly a year. Some unpublished experiments made in the author's laboratory have given different results.* Pigeons with only one canal cut recover practically completely within ten or more days. Those with two canals cut recover nearly completely within a month, so far as walking is concerned, although they exhibit an unwillingness to fly. Those with three or more canals cut never recover completely, but their final condition is very different from that exhibited shortly after the operation. Even when all six canals have been cut the animal, if well cared for in the beginning, is able finally to stand and walk and feed itself. It is not able, however, to fly, and in walking its progress is uncertain ; there is a tendency to walk zigzag or in circles, first to one side, then to the other. If hurried or excited some return of the violent movements of the head and ihco-ordination of the movements of locomotion may be seen. The partial recovery from the operations upon the canals may be due, however, to a more or less complete restoration of the canals to their former functional activity, owing to a regeneration, partial or complete, since a new section of the canals, after a year or more, again brings on the violent and dis- orderly movements of the head and the body. Effect of Direct Stimulation of the Canals. — The mem- branous canals or their ampullary enlargements have been stimu- lated by many observers and by many different methods — electrical, chemical, and mechanical. The results of electrical stimulation are not constant nor striking, but chemical and especially mechanical stimulation in the hands of many observers has called forth definite movements of head or eyes similar in a general way to those caused * Experiments lasting over two years made by Dr. E. Rosencrantz. SEMICIRCULAR CANALS AND THE VESTIBULE. 375 by section of the canal, but lasting, of course, for a short time only. In the dog-fish Lee* finds that pressure upon an ampulla causes movements of the eyes and fins such as would occur normally if the animal's body were rotated in the plane of the canal stimulated. Effect of Section of the Ampullary or the Acoustic Nerve. — Many of the older and newer observers have cut one or both of the acoustic nerves or destroyed the entire labyrinth on one or both sides. The effects described vary somewhat with the animals used, but, in general, section of the nerve on one side is followed by forced movements, especially of rolling movements around the long axis of the body. When the nerves are cut on both sides disturbances in the power to maintain equilibrium perfectly are more or less distinctly marked. In fishes (dog-fish) the animal may swim or come to rest in unusual positions, — on the back or side, for instance. Is the Effect of Section of the Canals Due to Stimulation? — The movements that result from section of one or more of the canals have been attributed by some authors to stimulations set up by the injury caused by the operation, and by others have been considered as a result of the falling out of the stimuli normally and constantly proceeding from the canals. This fundamental question has not been decided. On the one hand, the movements observed are simi- lar to those caused by excitation, which would indicate that a stimu- lation is set up by the operation. On the other hand, the effects are so long lasting as to make it improbable that they are entirely due to the irritation of the operation. Moreover, Gaglio f states that when the spot operated upon is cocainized the same effects follow. Indeed, cocainizing the membranous canals gives the same results as cutting them. It is possible, of course, that both processes take place, an irritative stimulation and a falling out of normal impulses, the effects of the latter being longer lasting. Theories of the Functions of the Semicircular Canals. — As indicated briefly above, the facts regarding injury to and stimulation of the semicircular canals are very numerous and, on the whole, fairly concordant. Their interpretation, however, has offered great dif- ficulties, and many views have been proposed; almost every inves- tigator, in fact, has, to some extent, varied in his interpretation of the precise functional significance of these organs. t These views may be classified, although imperfectly, under the following heads: 1. The old view, first proposed by Autenrieth (1802), that the canals or their sense cells are stimulated by sound waves and give * Lee, "Journal of Physiology/' 15, 328, 1903. t Gaglio, "Archives ital. de biologie," 31, 377, 1899. J For a detailed and complete account of these views to 1892 see Stein, "Die Lehren von den Funktionen der eizelnen Theile des Ohrlabyrinths." Jena, 1894. 376 THE SPECIAL SENSES. us the means of determining the direction of sound in accordance with their position in three planes at right angles to one another. This view has been revived from time to time by recent writers. 2. Flourens himself believed that the impulses normally proceed- ing from these organs serve to moderate, or, as we should say now, to inhibit the movements of the head. As soon as the canals are cut the movements that have been kept under control by their influence are unrestrained. On this view the semicircular canals are organs which inhibit or restrain the voluntary movements, and thus take an essential part in the proper co-ordination of such movements. He did not attempt to define the physiology of the organs in terms of the sensations aroused. 3. The view that the stimulus to the hair-cells is to be found in the varying pressure of the endolymph. As first proposed by Goltz (1870), it was assumed that the endolymph exerts a hydrostatic pressure upon the hair cells which in any given position varies in the different ampullas and varies with different positions of the head. The sensory impulses thus aroused give us a knowledge of the posi- tion of the head and enable us, therefore, to control its movements and also those of the body. On this view these organs act as sense organs in maintaining body equilibrium and may be designated as peripheral sense organs of equilibrium. Later observers (Mach, Breuer, Brown, et at.) modified this view by the assumption that the hair cells are stimulated not so much by the hydrostatic pressure of the endolymph as by the pressure changes developed during move- ments of the head, making the organs, therefore, a means of appreciating especially the movements of the head, a dynamic rather than a hydrostatic organ of equilibrium. It was assumed that rotation movements of the head in the plane of a canal set up a movement or pressure of the endolymph in the opposite direc- tion, just as, to use a rough comparison, when one twirls a pail of water in one direction the water lags behind and exerts a pressure in the opposite direction. According to this hypothesis, which in some form or other is the view usually taught, the hair cells in each ampulla are stimulated chiefly by movements in the plane of that canal toward the ampulla, the pressure of the endolymph be- ing in the opposite direction, — that is, from utriculus toward the canal. Moreover, the vertical canals act in pairs (see Fig. 167), the superior or anterior vertical of one side acting with the posterior or inferior vertical of the other side, the two canals lying in parallel planes. Movements in this plane forward would stimulate the anterior ampulla on one side chiefly, movements in the same plane backward, the posterior ampulla of the opposite side. The horizon- tal canals also act together, being stimulated chiefly by rotational movements in the horizontal plane, the hair cells in one responding SEMICIRCULAR CANALS AND THE VESTIBULE. 377 chiefly to movements in one direction, the other to movements in the same plane, but in the opposite direction. Rotational movements in other planes — sagittal, oblique, etc. — would affect two or more of the pairs of canals in proportion to the degree that each is involved in the movement on the principle of the parallelogram of forces.* By a mechanism of this sort it may be supposed that we are informed regarding the plane, di- rection, and extent of the movements of the head and are thereby enabled to control these movements. The canals function es- pecially as a dynamic organ of equilibrium, but may also give us guiding sensations when the movements are progressive rather than rotational, and also when the head is at rest, although, as is ex- plained below, this last function is by some relegated to the hair cells of the utriculus and sacculus. According to this view, the loss of the power of maintaining exact equilibrium after injuries to the canals or section of the nerves may be explained by supposing that false sensations are experienced and false compensatory move- ments are made. So, also, the vertigo experienced after continued rotation may be attributed to abnormal stimulation of these sense organs, — a view that finds some support in the fact that many deaf-mutes, whose internal ear is supposed to be deficient, do not experience vertigo after rotation, and in animals with the labyrinth destroyed rotational movements fail to give the symptoms of vertigo. 4. Cyon has advocated the view that the semicircular canals con- stitute an organ for the perception of space in its three dimensions. Each canal or pair of canals gives us the sense of direction in its own plane, and the fact that we have three pairs in planes at right angles to one another gives the physiological foundation of our conception of three dimensional space. On this fundamental conception of space is projected the additional space conceptions derived from our visual, tactile, and muscle senses. This author is not specific in stating by what means the sensory cells in the three canals are stimulated. In addition to the sensations of direction and of space furnished by the canals, the nerve impulses from them are supposed to co-ordinate the action of the motor centers concerned in move- ments of the head and body. 5. Ewald, while accepting the general view that the sense cells are stimulated by the pressure of the endolymph, lays stress upon the fact that the nerve impulses thus aroused have, as their main result, a reflex effect upon the tonicity of the voluntary muscula- ture. The constant flow of impulses from these organs serves to maintain the muscles in a normal condition of tone. In animals with the labyrinth destroyed on both sides the body musculature is flabby and lacking in tonicity. On this view, therefore, the semi- * Consult Lee, loc. cit. 378 THE SPECIAL SENSES. circular canals constitute what might be called a muscle-tone organ, and the obvious disturbances in motion caused by their injury are due primarily to a diminution or loss in muscle tone, each canal possibly being reflexly connected with special muscles. Summary. — With reference to the kind of sensation mediated by the nerves of the semicircular canals, it should be borne in mind that these sensations are not distinctly recognized by consciousness; hence the difficulty of designating them by a specific name. Of the many qualities of sensation or consciousness which we can distinguish some have characteristics so clear that we recognize them at once and give them distinctive names, — such, for instance, as the sensations of sight, hearing, taste, etc. Others, however, produce a psychical reaction of such an indefinite character that they escape recognition by mere introspection. The change in con- sciousness is not sufficiently marked to make itself felt to the un- trained mind. This condition prevails regarding the sensations aroused through the semicircular canals ; they are too indistinct to be recognized and named by an appeal to consciousness, and it would seem to be wiser to designate them after the analogy of the muscle sensations simply as semicircular canal sensations. Our perceptions or ideas of space and direction are doubtless founded in part upon these reactions and in part upon the muscle sense, vision, and tactile sensations. With regard to the influence of the nerve impulses from the semicircular canals upon movements, all the facts known seem to indicate that they play an important part in the regulation or co-ordination of the movements of equilibrium and locomotion. Inasmuch as this general co-ordination or con- trol seems to rest normally in the nervous mechanisms of the cere- bellum and inasmuch as the vestibular nerves make end connections with the cerebellum, together with the fibers of muscle sense, we may assume that the cerebellum forms the brain center in which the semicircular canal impulses exert their influence upon muscular contractions and muscle tone, — the cerebellum forms the nerve center for the semicircular canals, or the semicircular canals form a periph- eral sense organ to the cerebellum. Whether the impulses from , the canals are excitatory or inhibitory or both, as regards their effect upon muscular contractions, is not clearly apparent from the experimental evidence so far furnished, but Ewald's suggestion that they serve to maintain reflexly the tonus of the body muscu- lature is perhaps the most acceptable view. In regard to the means by which these nerves are normally stimulated there is also much room for conjecture, but provisionally at least it seems permissible to adopt the view that variations in the pressure of the endolymph upon the hairs of the hair cells constitute the immediate cause of their excitation. Granting that changes in position or SEMICIRCULAR CANALS AND THE VESTIBULE. 379 movement of the head may cause such variations in pressure the theory offers a simple and satisfactory explanation of the mode of excitation and the means by which the excitation may vary appropriately under different conditions. While the endolymph theory may be criticized easily, no other equally satisfactory theory has been suggested to take its place. Functions of the Utriculus and Sacculus. — These small sacs contain sensory hair cells similar in general structure to those found in the crista of the ampullary sacs. The collection of hair cells with their supporting cells is designated as the macula, the macula utriculi and the macula sacculi. Lying among the hairs of the hair cell are found masses of small crystals of calcium carbonate, the otoliths or otoconia. In this respect the structure of the macula differs strik- ingly from that of the crista. The position and connections of the utriculus and sacculus lead at first naturally to the supposition that they are stimulated by the sound waves of the perilymph and are, therefore, concerned in the function of hearing. The accepted views regarding the functions of the cochlea in hearing make this organ sufficient for all auditory purposes and there is no specific part of this process that need be attributed to the vestibular sacs. It was, indeed, at one time suggested that their structure adapts them to respond especially to short and irregular vibrations, but no cogent reasons or facts have been advanced to support this view. The fact that the sacs are so closely connected with the semicircular canals suggests rather that the functions of these organs are similar and that like the canals, therefore, they influence the contractions of the muscles and function as organs of equilibrium. In recent years the view that has been most discussed is that ad- vanced by Breuer, — namely, that these organs give us information regarding the position of the head when at rest and when mak- ing progressive — that is, non-rotary — movements, supplementing, therefore, the functions of the semicircular canals on the supposition that these latter act especially in movements of rotation. Or, as it is sometimes expressed, the sacs form a static and the canals a dy- namic organ of equilibrium. According to this view, the otoliths act as a means of mechanical stimulation of the hairs. Being heavier than the endolymph, they press upon the hairs with a force varying with the position of the head and thus give rise to sensations or reflexes which are adapted to the maintenance of equilibrium. Since the planes of the two sacs are different, they may be differ- ently affected by the same position or movement. So also in pro- gressive movements forward the weight of the otoliths may be im- agined to exercise a stress of some sort upon the hairs. This theory has been the subject of much investigation, numerous experiments 380 THE SPECIAL SENSES. having been made chiefly upon fishes and invertebrates.* Accord- ing to some observers destruction of these sacs or section of their nerves is accompanied by a distinct interference with the fish's nor- mal equilibrium: the animal swims at times upon his back or side and apparently loses its normal means of judging correctly its posi- tion. In many inver tebrates there is present a sac, known as the otocyst, containing hair cells and otoliths. Its structure resembles that of the vestibular sacs of the mammalian ear, and it has been assumed that it has a similar function. Experiments by numerous observers have indicated that when the otoliths are removed the animal shows disturbances in equilibrium, particularly in the matter of the compensatory movements exhibited during rotation. Others, however, deny these facts and state that invertebrates without oto- cysts make compensatory movements when rotated and that in those with otocysts compensatory movements and maintenance of normal equilibrium persist after destruction of the sacs. A very ingenious experiment reported by Kreidl seems to show that the oto- liths may affect the hairs by their weight. When the palaemon, a crustacean, molts it casts off the inner lining of the otocyst, together with the otoliths. The otocysts in these animals lie at the base of the antennules and open freely to the exterior. After molting the animal by means of its claws places fine grains of sand in the otocyst to act as otoliths. Taking advantage of this peculiarity, Kreidl placed the animal, after molting, upon finely powdered iron, with the result that some of the iron granules were deposited in the oto- cyst in place of the usual grains of sand. When now a magnet was brought near to the animal reactions were obtained which showed that the pressure of the iron upon the hairs influenced its position. The position taken by the animal under these conditions was such as would be expected as a resultant of the forces of magnetism and gravity, and the experiment, therefore, justifies the hypothesis that under normal conditions gravity affects the otoliths and through them the muscular co-ordination of the animal. * Consult the following papers: Sewall, "Journal of Physiology," 4, 339, 1884; Lee, ibid., 15, 311, 1893, and "American Journal of Physiology," 1, 128, 1898; Lyon, "American Journal of Physiology," 3, 86, 1900. SECTION IV. BLOOD AND LYMPH. CHAPTER XXII. GENERAL PROPERTIES: PHYSIOLOGY OF THE CORPUSCLES. The blood of the body is contained in a practically closed system of tubes, the blood-vessels, within which it is kept circulating by the force of the heart beat. It is usually spoken of as the nutritive liquid of the body, but its functions may be stated more explicitly, al- though still in quite general terms, by saying that it carries to the tis- sues foodstuffs after they have been properly prepared by the diges- tive organs ; that it transports to the tissues oxygen absorbed from the air in the lungs; that it carries off from the tissues various waste products formed in the processes of disassimilation ; that it is the medium for the transmission of the internal secretion of certain glands; and that it aids in equalizing the temperature and water contents of the body. It is quite obvious, from these statements, that a complete consideration of the physiological relations of the blood would involve substantially a treatment of the whole subject of physiology. It is proposed, therefore, in this section to treat the blood in a restricted way, — to consider it, in fact, as a tissue in itself, and to study its composition and properties without special reference to its nutritive relationship to other parts of the body. Histological Structure. — The blood is composed of a liquid part, the plasma, in which float a vast number of microscopical bodies, the blood corpuscles. There are at least three different kinds of cor- puscles, known respectively as the red corpuscles; the white corpus- cles or leucocytes, of which in turn there are a number of different kinds; and the blood plates. Blood-plasma, when obtained free from corpuscles, is perfectly colorless in thin layers, — for example, in microscopical preparations; when seen in large quantities it shows a slightly yellowish tint, the depth of color varying with different animals. The red color of blood is not due, therefore, to coloration of the blood-plasma, but is caused by the mass of red corpuscles held 381 382 BLOOD AND LYMPH. in suspension in this liquid. The proportion by bulk of plasma to corpuscles is usually given, roughly, as two to one. Blood-serum and Defibrinated Blood. — In connection with the explanation of the term "blood-plasma " just given it will be con- venient to define briefly the terms " blood-serum " and " defibrin- ated blood." Blood, after it escapes from the vessels, usually clots or coagulates; the nature of this process is discussed in detail on page 415. The clot, as it forms, gradually shrinks and squeezes out a clear liquid to which the name blood-serum is given. Serum re- sembles the plasma of normal blood in general appearance, but dif- fers from it in composition, as will be explained later. At present we may say, by way of a preliminary definition, that blood-serum is the liquid part of blood after coagulation has taken place, as blood- plasma is the liquid part of blood before coagulation has taken place. If shed blood is whipped vigorously with a rod or some similar object while it is clotting, the essential part of the clot — namely, the fibrin — forms differently from what it does when the blood is allowed to coagulate quietly ; it is deposited in shreds on the whipper. Blood that has been treated in this way is known as defibrinated blood. It consists of blood-serum plus the red and white corpuscles, and as far as appearances go it resembles exactly normal blood; it has lost, however, the power of clotting. A more complete definition of these terms will be given after the subject of coagulation has been treated. Reaction of the Blood. — When tested with litmus or lakmoid paper blood gives an alkaline reaction. This reaction is attributed to the sodium carbonate in solution in the plasma, and the amount of the alkalinity has been determined, therefore, by titration with a weak acid, such as tartaric acid. The acid is employed in a known strength — one two-hundredth or one four-hundredth normal solution, — ^ or ~} that is the solution contains in each liter i or ™ of the number of grams represented by the molecular weight of tartaric acid (C4H606= 150). A solution of this strength is added to a known quantity of blood until the sodium carbonate is all neutralized, the end of the reaction being determined usually by one of the recognized indicators, such as litmus or lakmoid. Tested in this way, it has been found that the alkalinity of the blood corresponds to that of an aqueous solution containing from 0.2 to 0.3 per cent, of sodium carbonate. Much attention has been paid to the variations in the alkalinity of the blood in different diseases, as also under varying conditions of normal life, and in consequence many methods for determining this alkalinity have been suggested with reference to their clinical application, these methods being characterized by the fact that but little blood is employed. The GENERAL PROPERTIES: THE CORPUSCLES. 383 methods differ among themselves chiefly in the means used to determine the point of neutralization of the blood by the acid added. In some methods litmus is employed, in others lakmoid, and in one (Dare's) the end-reaction is determined spectroscopically on the belief that the characteristic absorption spectrum of oxy- hemoglobin (p. 395) disappears at the point bf neutralization.* In reference to this subject of the reaction of the blood and of the tissues of the body generally a difference in terminology prevails at present which tends to confuse the beginner. Some writers use the term alkalinity in the sense of titration alkalinity to indicate that the blood will neutralize a certain amount of weak acid added to it. Others, however, employ the term in its strict sense, as developed by modern physical chemistry, to indicate an excess of hydroxyl ions (OH — ). From the latter standpoint a solution is alkaline when it contains a substance or substances which upon dissociation yield an excess of hydroxyl ions, — sodium hydroxid, for example, which on dissociation gives Na+ and OH — , or sub- stances, such as sodium carbonate, which give rise to hydroxyl ions by reaction with water. In a solution of this latter salt we may assume that some of the molecules dissociate into the ions Na + , Na+ , and C03= , and that the anion, C03= , reacts with the disso- ciated molecules of water, H+, HO — , giving HC03 — and OH — . There will be present in the solution, therefore, the following ions, Na+ , OH — , and Na+ , HC03 — ; and the presence of the hydroxyl ion confers upon the solution its alkaline reaction and properties. In such a solution of a strong base with a weak acid the alkalinity, that is, excess of OH — , can not be determined by titration with an acid stronger than carbonic acid. If tartaric acid is added, for instance, the acid will not only give its H+ to combine with the OH — , but its own anion will combine with all of the dissociated Na-f- ; consequently more Na3C03 will be dissociated, and this reaction goes on, if sufficient acid is used, until all of the sodium carbonate is destroyed. To determine the excess of hydroxyl ions in such a solution as blood it is neces- sary to make use of the methods of physical chemistry. Those who have employed these methodsf report that blood contains no greater quantity of hydroxyl ions than pure water, and must, therefore, be reckoned as a neutral liquid. This conclusion is corroborated further by the fact that with some indicators — e. g., phenolphthalein — the blood does not give an alkaline reac- tion. In fact, the sodium in the blood behaves substantially as if it were present as the bicarbonate, NaHC03, — a theoretically acid salt whose dis- sociation would be represented by the two ions Na+, HC03 — . The many observations, therefore, which have been made by titration of the blood must be considered as not giving its variations in alkalinity, but rather the varia- tions in the amount of alkali, Na, in combination with weak acids, such as car- bonate or phosphate. The results represent what has been called the "titra- tion alkalinity ' of the blood. Specific Gravity. — The specific gravity of human blood in the adult male may vary from 1.041 to 1.067, the average being about * For an account of these methods see Simon, "A Manual of Clinical Diag- nosis," 1904. f Fraenckel, "Archiv f. d. gesammte Physiologic," 96, 601, 1903; and Hober, ibid., 99, 572. 384 BLOOD AND LYMPH. 1.055. The most satisfactory method of determining this factor is, of course, to compare the weight of a known volume of blood with that of an equal volume of water, but for observations upon human beings such small quantities of blood must be used that recourse must be had usually to a more indirect method. Perhaps the simplest of the methods suggested is that devised by Hammerschlag.* In this method a mixture is made of chloroform (sp. gr., 1.526) and benzol (sp. gr., 0.889). The mixture is made in such proportions as to have a specific gravity of about 1.055. A drop of blood from the finger is shaken into this mixture; if the drop sinks to the bottom it is evident that the specific gravity of the blood is higher than that of the mixture, and the reverse is true if the drop rises. By adding more of the chloroform or of the benzol, as the case may be, the specific gravity of the mixture may be quickly altered so as to be equal to that of the drop of blood, which will then float in the liquid without a distinct tendency to rise or fall. The specific gravity of the mixture, which is also that of the blood, is then determined by a suitable hydrometer. By the use of such methods it has been found f that the specific gravity varies with age and with sex; that it is diminished after eating and is increased after exercise; that it has a diurnal variation, falling gradually during the day and rising slowly during the night; and that it varies greatly in individuals, so that a specific gravity which is normal for one may be a sign of disease in another. The specific gravity of the corpuscles is slightly greater than that of the plasma. For this reason the corpuscles in shed blood, when its coagulation is prevented or retarded, tend to settle to the bottom of the containing utensil, leaving a more or less clear layer of supernatant plasma. Among themselves, also, the cor- puscles differ slightly in specific gravity, the red corpuscles being heaviest and the blood plates lightest. Red Corpuscles. — The red corpuscles in man and in all the mammalia, with the exception of the camel and other members of the group Camelidse, are biconcave circular discs without nuclei; in the Camelidse they have an elliptical form. Their average diame- ter in man is given as 7.7 // (1 [i = 0.001 mm.) ; their number, which is usually reckoned as so many in a cubic millimeter, varies greatly under different conditions of health and disease. The average number is given as 5,000,000 per c.mm. for males and 4,500,000 for females. The red color of the corpuscles is due to the presence in them of a pigment known as "hemoglobin." Owing to the minute size of the corpuscles, their color when seen singly under the micro- scope is a faint yellowish red, but when seen in mass they exhibit the well-known blood-red color, which varies from scarlet in arterial * Hammerschlag, "Zeitschrift f. klin. Med.," 20, 444, 1892. f See Jones, " Journal of Physiology," 12, 299, 1891. GENERAL PROPERTIES: THE CORPUSCLES. 385 blood to purplish red in venous blood, this variation in color being dependent upon the amount of oxygen contained in the blood in combination with the hemoglobin. Speaking generally, the func- tion of the red corpuscles is to carry oxygen from the lungs to the tissues. This function is, entirely dependent upon the presence of hemoglobin, which has the power of combining easily with oxygen gas. The physiology of the red corpuscles, therefore, is largely con- tained in a description of the properties of hemoglobin. Condition of the Hemoglobin m the Corpuscle. — The finer structure of the red corpuscle is not completely known. It is usually stated that the corpuscle is composed of two substances, stroma and hem- oglobin, together with a certain amount of water and salts and also small amounts of lecithin and cholesterin. The stroma is a delicate, extensible, colorless substance that gives shape to the corpuscles; it forms a meshwork or spongy mass in which the hemoglobin is deposited. This latter substance forms the chief constituent of the corpuscle, since it makes about 32 per cent, of the weight of the normal corpuscle, and when dry from 90 to 95 per cent, of the total solid material. The point that remains uncertain is the condition in which the hemoglobin exists within the corpuscle. It is evidently not in solution, since the amount present is too great to be held in solution in the corpuscle, and, moreover even a thin layer of corpuscles is far from being transparent. Nor is it deposited in the form of crystals. It is assumed, therefore, that it is present in a peculiar, amorphous form, and Gamgee has shown that from its aqueous solutions the hemoglobin can be obtained in an amor- phous state by the action of an electrical current. It is protected from the action of the water, within and without the corpuscle. In various ways, however, the relations of the hemoglobin within the corpuscle may be disturbed; so that it escapes and enters into solution in the plasma. Blood in which this has happened suffers a change in color, becoming a dark crimson, and is therefore known as "laked blood." Laked blood in thin layers is quite transparent compared with the normal blood with its opaque corpuscles. Hemolysis. — The act of discharging the hemoglobin from the corpuscles so that it becomes dissolved in the plasma is designated as hemolysis, and substances that cause this action are spoken of as hemolytic agents. A number of such agents are known; but, although the results of their action are the same, so far as the hemo- globin is concerned, the way in which they bring about this result must vary greatly. Some of the known methods of producing hemolysis, or rendering the blood "laky," are as follows: (1) By the addition of water to the blood or by diminishing in any way the concentration or osmotic pressure in the plasma. (2) By add- ing ether or chloroform. (3) By adding bile or solutions of the bile 25 386 BLOOD AND LYMPH. salts. (4) By adding amyl-alcohol. (5) By adding the serum from the blood of certain animals. (6) By adding saponin or sapotoxin. (7) By the addition of an excess of alkali. (8) By various toxins found in snake venom or in the serum of other animals or among the products of bacterial activity (natural hemolysins) or by similar organic substances produced within the body by the process of immunizing. Two of these methods de- mand especial mention, as they involve the consideration of proc- esses of great physiological importance. Hemolysis Caused by Lowering the Osmotic Pressure of the Plasma. — The blood corpuscles contain a certain amount of water ( 57 to 64 per cent.), an amount insufficient to discharge the hemoglobin. We may imagine that the osmotic pressure within the corpuscle is such, compared With the osmotic pressure exerted by the salts in the plasma, that a water equilibrium is established, and that, although water molecules diffuse into and out of the corpuscle, the exchange is equal in the two directions. If, however, the outside plasma is diluted by the addition of water to any consider- able extent, then the osmotic pressure outside the corpuscles is correspondingly reduced, while that within the corpuscles is unchanged. Consequently an increased amount of water will pass into the corpuscles, sufficient, in fact, to discharge and dissolve the hemoglobin. It is evident, therefore, that, in injecting liquids into the circulation, or in diluting blood outside the body, care must be taken not to use solutions whose osmotic pressure is markedly less than that of blood-plasma, otherwise many of the red corpuscles may be destroyed. Solutions whose osmotic pressure is the same as that of the plasma are said to be isosmotic or isotonic with the blood, those whose pressure is lower are designated as hypotonic, and those whose pressure is higher as hypertonic* The salt that is contained in the plasma in largest amounts is sodium chlorid. In making isotonic solutions this salt is there- fore generally employed. A solution containing nine- tenths of 1 per cent, of sodium chlorid (NaCl, 0.9 per cent.) gives the same osmotic pressure as plasma as determined by the effect of each on the lowering of the freezing point (see appendix, Diffusion, Osmosis, and Osmotic Pressure). Such a solution mixed with blood should not and does not alter the water contents of the corpuscles. One may, in fact, use a 0.7 per cent, solution of sodium chlorid with- out causing any noticeable hemolysis, and this strength of solution is generally employed in infusions and experimental work; it con- stitutes what is known in the laboratories as normal saline or physio- * For a full consideration of osmotic pressure in its relations to physio- logical processes, see Hamburger, "Osmotischer Druck und Ionenlehre." Wiesbaden, 1902. GENERAL PROPERTIES*. THE CORPUSCLES. 387 logical saline. If, however, one uses a lower concentration some of the corpuscles are hemolyzed, and the number of corpuscles destroyed and the rapidity of the hemolysis increase rapidly with the lowering of the osmotic pressure. While a 0.7 per cent, solution of sodium chiorid suffices in most cases for infusions and for diluting blood, it does not entirely replace the normal plasma or serum, since these liquids, in addition to the sodium salts, contain salts of calcium, potassium, magnesium, etc., each of which has doubtless a certain specific importance. In diluting blood outside the body, when the dilution is large, better results are obtained by using what is known as Ringer's mixture, which consists of the physiological saline solution plus small amounts of potassium and calcium chiorid. One formula for Ringer's solution is: Sodium chiorid 0.7 per cent. Calcium chiorid 0.026 " " Potassium chiorid 0.03 " " Hemolysis Caused by the Action of Hemolysins. — It has long been known that the serum of one animal may destroy the red corpuscles of another animal. Thus, rabbits' blood corpuscles added to the clear serum of a dog, cat, or man are quickly destroyed, with the liberation of their hemoglobin. This action was formerly described under the term tl globulicidal action of serum," and was compared to the similar destruction, bactericidal action, exhibited by serum toward some bacteria. In more recent literature the term hemol- ysis has replaced that of " globulicidal action," and the hemolytic effect that a serum may exert upon foreign corpuscles is attributed to the presence in it of certain substances which in general are classed as hemolysins. This hemolytic action is not due to a simple differ- ence in osmotic pressure. The serums of the different mammalia have all approximately the same osmotic pressure; the differences are too slight to explain the effects observed. Moreover, if the serum used is heated to 55° C. its hemolytic action is destroyed, although no noticeable change occurs in the osmotic pressure. In addition to the hemolysins found normally in the blood of different animals it was shown first by Bordet * that they may be produced artificially. The serum of guinea pigs has little or no effect normally on the red corpuscles of rabbits' blood. If, however, one injects into a guinea pig a little rabbits' blood and repeats the process several times at intervals of a day or so it will be found that the blood of this particular guinea pig has now a strong hemolytic action toward the red corpuscles of rabbits. Moreover, the action is specific, — that is, the serum is hemolytic only toward the red corpuscles of the species whose blood was injected into it. The process of producing a specific * Bordet, "Annales de lTnst. Pasteur," 1895. 388 BLOOD AND LYMPH. hemolysin by injections is designated usually as immunization, on account of its essential similarity to the means used to produce a specific antitoxin by injecting a given toxin, — that is, of rendering an animal immune. The specific hemolysins produced by immuniza- tion have been studied by Bordet, Ehrlich, and others.* It has been shown that they are in reality composed of two substances whose combined action is necessary for the hemolysis. There is, first, a new and specific substance that is produced by the body as a conse- quence of the injection of the foreign blood corpuscles. This sub- stance has been given different names, but is known most frequently as the immune body (or amboceptor). It is not destroyed by mod- erate heating. The immune body is enabled to act upon the cor- puscles by the co-operation of certain substances which are normally present in the serum and are therefore not produced by the process of immunization. These substances are known usually as comple- ments, and it is they that are destroyed by heating to 55° C. If the immune serum of a guinea pig is heated to 55° C. its hemolytic action upon rabbits' corpuscles is destroyed. The action may be restored, however, by adding a little of the rabbit's own serum, since in terms of the above hypothesis the complements are present in normal serum. The results obtained from the study of these bodies are of extraordinary interest in connection with the subject of acquired immunity toward various diseases, and also in the evidence they furnish of the wonderful complexity of the reactions exhibited by living organisms. For it is found that specific substances, lysins, capable of destroying special kinds of cells, may be produced by the injection of spermatozoa, epithelial cells, etc. Any foreign cell in- troduced into the body seems to call forth the production of an immune body capable of destroying only that particular kind of cell. Poisons or toxins produced in this way may be designated in general as cytotoxins, — that is, cell poisons, — and it is believed that specific toxins are produced or may be produced for each kind of cell. Im- munizing with spermatozoa gives rise to a spermotoxin in the blood of the animal immunized, while injection of red corpuscles causes the formation of a hemotoxin, or, as it is usually called in this case, hemolysin. As the chemical nature of this reaction is beyond our present knowledge, it is designated frequently as the biological reac- tion of the living substance. In the case of some of the natural hemolysins referred to above it has also been shown that they are composed in reality of two bodies, each necessary to the reaction, — one the complement, destroyed by heating, and one comparable to * For a brief statement of the development of the subject, see Wasser- mann, "Immune Sera, Hemolysins, Cytotoxins, and Precipitins," trans- lated by Bolduan, New York, 1904. For a more extended review see Aschoff, "Zeitschrift f. allgemeine Physiologie," 1,69, 1902. GENERAL PROPERTIES: THE CORPUSCLES. 389 the immune body, but in this case designated as the interbody or intermediary body, since it is not produced by immunization. Speaking in general terms, the serum of any animal is more or less hemolytic in regard to the blood corpuscles of an animal of another species; but great differences are shown in this respect. The blood-serum of the horse shows but little hemolytic action upon the red corpuscles of the rabbit when compared with the effect of the serum of the dog or cat. Eels* serum has a remarkably strong hemolytic action upon the red corpuscles of most mammals ; a very minute quantity of this serum (0.04 c.c.) injected into the veins of a rabbit will cause hemolysis of the corpuscles and, as a consequence, the appearance of bloody urine (hemoglobinuria). It should be added that this curious toxic or lytic effect of foreign serums is not confined to the red corpuscles. They contain cytotoxins that affect also other tissue elements, especially those of the central nervous system, and may therefore cause death. As little as 0.04 c.c. of eels' serum injected into a small rabbit will cause the death of the animal, the fatal effect being due, apparently to an action on the vasomotor and respiratory center in the medulla. The hemolytic and generally toxic effect of foreign sera has been known for a long time. It was discovered practically in the numerous attempts made in former years to transfuse the blood of one animal into the veins of another. It has been found that this process of transfusion as a means of combatting severe hemorrhage is dangerous unless the blood is taken from an animal of the same or a nearly related species. Nature and Amount of Hemoglobin. — Hemoglobin is a very complex substance belonging to the group of combined proteids. Under the influence of heat, acids, alkalies, etc., it may be broken up, with the formation of a simple proteid, globin, belonging to the group of histons (see appendix) and a pigment, hematin. The globin forms, according to different estimates, from 86 to 94 per cent, of the molecule, and the hematin about 4 per cent. Other sub- stances of an undetermined character result from the decomposition.* When the decomposition takes place in the absence of oxygen, the products formed are globin and hemochromogen, instead of globin and hematin. Hemochromogen in the presence of oxygen quickly undergoes oxidation to the more stable hematin. Hoppe-Seyler has shown that hemochromogen possesses the chemical grouping which gives to hemoglobin its power of combining readily with oxy- gen and its distinctive absorption spectrum. On the basis of facts such as these, hemoglobin may be defined as a compound of a proteid body with hematin. It seems, then, that, although the hemochro- mogen or hematin portion is the essential thing, giving to the mole- * Schulz, "Zeitschrift f. physiologische Chemie," 24; also Lauraw, ibid., 26. 390 BLOOD AND LYMPH. cule of hemoglobin its valuable physiological properties as a respira- tory pigment, yet in the blood corpuscles this substance is incorpo- rated into the much larger and more unstable molecule of hemo- globin, whose behavior toward oxygen is different from that of the hematin itself, the difference lying mainly in the fact that the hemo- globin as it exists in the corpuscles forms with oxygen a compara- tively feeble combination that may be broken up readily, with liber- ation of the gas. Hemoglobin is widely distributed throughout the animal king- dom, being found in the blood corpuscles of mammalia, birds, reptiles, amphibia, and fishes, and in the blood or blood corpuscles of many of the invertebrates. The composition of its molecule is found to vary somewhat in different animals; so that, strictly speaking, there are probably a number of different forms of hemo- globin— all, however, closely related in chemical and physiological properties. Elementary analysis of dogs' hemoglobin shows the following percentage composition (Jaquet): C, 53.91; H, 6.62; N, 15.98; S, 0.542; Fe, 0.333; O, 22.62. Its molecular formula is given as C758H1203N195S3FeO218, which would make the molec- ular weight 16,669. Other estimates are given of the molecular formula, but they agree at least in showing that the molecule is of enormous size. The hematin that is split off from the hemoglobin is a pigment whose constitution is relatively simple, as is indicated by its percentage formula, C34H34N4Fe05 (Kiister). It contains all of the iron of the original hemoglobin molecule. Gamgee has called attention to two facts which seem to indicate that the globin and hematin do not exist as such in the hemoglobin molecule. Thus, hematin is magnetic, — that is, is attracted by a magnet, — while hemoglobin, on the contrary, is diamagnetic. Globin alone rotates the plane of polarized light to the left, levorotatory, while hemo- globin solutions are dextrorotatory. The exact amount of hemo- globin in human blood varies naturally with the individual and with different conditions of life. According to Preyer,* the average amount for the adult male is 14 grams of hemoglobin to each 100 grams of blood. It is estimated that in the blood of a man weighing 68 kilograms there are contained about 750 grams of hemoglobin, which is distributed among some 25,000,000,000,000 of corpuscles, giving a total superficial area of about 3200 square meters. Practi- cally all of this large surface of hemoglobin is available for the absorption of oxygen from the air in the lungs, for, owing to the great number and the minute size of the capillaries, the blood, in passing through a capillary area, becomes subdivided to such an extent that the red corpuscles stream through the capillaries, one may say, in single file. In circulating through the lungs, therefore, *"Die Blutkrystalle." Jena, 1871. GENERAL PROPERTIES: THE CORPUSCLES. 391 each corpuscle becomes exposed more or less completely to the action of the air, and the utilization of the entire quantity of hemoglobin must be nearly perfect. It may be worth while to call attention to the fact that the biconcave form of the red corpuscle increases the superficies of the corpuscle and tends to make the surface exposure of the hemoglobin more complete. Instruments known as hemometers or hemoglobinometers have been devised for clinical use in determining the amount of hemoglobin in the blood of patients. A number of different forms of this instrument are in use. In all of them, however, the determination is made with a drop or two of blood, such as can be obtained without difficulty by pricking the skin. The amount of hemoglobin in the withdrawn blood is determined usually by a colorimetric method, — that is, its color, which is due to the hemoglobin, is compared with a series of standard solutions containing known amounts of hemoglobin, or with a wedge of colored glass whose color value in terms of hemo- globin has been determined beforehand. For details of the structure of the several instruments employed and the precautions to be observed in their use reference must be made to the laboratory guides.* Compounds with Oxygen and other Gases. — Hemoglobin has the property of uniting with oxygen gas in certain definite propor- tions, forming a true chemical compound. This compound is known as oxyhemoglobin-, it is formed whenever blood or hemoglobin solutions are exposed to air or otherwise brought into contact with oxygen. Each molecule of hemoglobin is supposed to combine with one molecule of oxygen. According to a determination by Hufner,f the O capacity of the Hb of ox's blood is 1.34 c.c. O to each gram of Hb. Oxyhemoglobin is not a very firm compound. If placed in an atmosphere containing no oxygen, it is dissociated, giving off free oxygen and leaving behind hemoglobin, or, as it is often called by way of distinction, "reduced hemoglobin." This power of com- bining with oxygen to form a loose chemical compound, which in turn can be dissociated easily when the oxygen pressure is lowered, makes possible the function of hemoglobin in the blood as the carrier of of oxygen from the lungs to the tissues. The details of this process are described in the section on Respiration. Hemoglobin forms with carbon monoxid gas (CO) a compound, similar to oxyhemo- globin, which is known as carbon monoxid hemoglobin. In this compound also the union takes place in the proportion of one molecule of hemoglobin to one molecule of the gas. The compound formed differs, however, from oxyhemoglobin in being much more stable, and it is for this reason that the breathing of carbon monoxid *See Simon, "A Manual of Clinical Diagnosis." Philadelphia, 1904. f "Archiv f. Physiologie," 1894, p. 130. 392 BLOOD AND LYMPH. gas is liable to prove fatal. The CO unites with the hemoglobin, forming a firm compound; the tissues of the body are thereby pre- vented from obtaining their necessary oxygen, and death results from suffocation or asphyxia. Carbon monoxid forms one of the constituents of coal-gas. The well-known fatal effect of breathing coal-gas for some time, as in the case of individuals sleeping in a room in which gas is escaping, is traceable directly to the carbon monoxid. Nitric oxid (NO) forms also with hemoglobin a definite compound that is even more stable than the CO hemoglobin; if, therefore, this gas were brought into contact with the blood, it would cause death in the same way as the CO. Oxyhemoglobin, carbon monoxid hemoglobin, and nitric oxid hemoglobin are similar compounds. Each is formed, apparently, by a definite combination of the gas with the hematin portion of the hemoglobin molecule, and a given weight of hemoglobin unites presumably with an equal volume of each gas. In marked contrast to these facts, Bohr* has shown that hemoglobin forms a compound with carbon dioxid gas, carbohemoglobin, in which the quantitative relationship of the gas to the hemoglobin differs from that shown by oxygen. In a mixture of O and C02 the latter gas is absorbed by hemoglobin solutions independently of the oxygen, so that a solu- tion of hemoglobin nearly saturated with oxygen will take up C02 as though it held no oxygen in combination. Bohr suggests, there- fore, that the O and the C02 must unite with different portions of the hemoglobin — the oxygen with the pigment portion and the C02 possi- bly with the proteid portion. Although the amount of C02 taken up by the hemoglobin is not influenced by the amount of O already in combination, the reverse relationship does not hold in all cases. It is found that the presence of the C02 loosens, as it were, the combina- tion between the hemoglobin and the oxygen so that the oxyhemo- globin dissociates more readily than would otherwise be the case. This is observed at least when the oxygen is under a low pressure, such as occurs, for instance, in the capillaries of the tissues. The importance of this fact in regard to the oxygen supply to the tissues is referred to more explicitly in the section on Respiration. Presence of Iron in the Molecule. — It is probable that iron is quite generally present in the animal tissues in connection with nuclein compounds, but its existence in hemoglobin is noteworthy because it has long been known, and because the important property of combining with oxygen seems to be connected with the presence of this element. According to the analyses made, the proportion of iron in hemoglobin varies somewhat in different animals: the figures given are from 0.333 to 0.47 per cent. The amount of hemoglobin in blood may be determined, therefore, by making a * " Skandinavisches Archiv f. Physiologie," 3, 47, 1892, and 16, 402, 1904. GENERAL PROPERTIES: THE CORPUSCLES. 393 quantitative determination of the iron. The amount of oxygen with which hemoglobin will combine may be expressed by saying that one molecule of oxygen will be fixed for each atom of iron in the hemoglobin molecule. In the decomposition of hemoglobin into globin and hematin, which has been spoken of above, the iron is retained in the hematin. Crystals. — Hemoglobin may be obtained readily in the form of crystals (Fig. 168). As usually prepared, these crystals are really oxyhemoglobin, but it has been shown that reduced hemoglobin also crystallizes, although with more diffi- culty. Hemoglobin from the blood of different ani- mals varies to a marked degree in respect to the power of crystallization. From the blood of the rat, dog, cat, guinea pig, and horse, crystals are readily obtained, while hemoglobin from the blood of man and of most of the vertebrates crystallizes much less easily. Methods for preparing and purifying these crystals will be found in works on phys- iological chemistry. To ob- tain specimens quickly for examination under the mi- croscope, one of the most certain methods is to take some blood from one of the animals whose hemoglobin crystallizes easily, place it in a test-tube, add to it a few drops of ether, shake the tube thoroughly until the blood be- comes laky, — that is, until the hemoglobin is discharged into the plasma, — and then place the tube on ice until the crystals are deposited. Small portions of the crystalline sediment may then be removed to a glass slide for examination. According to Reichert, the deposition of the crystals is hastened by adding ammonium oxalate to the blood in quantities sufficient to make from 1 to 5 per cent, of the mixture. Hemoglobin from different animals varies not only as to the ease with which it crystallizes, but in some 168. — Crystallized hemoglobin (after b, Crystals from venous blood of man; Fig. Frey): a c, from the blood of a cat; d, from the blood of a guinea pig; e, from the blood of a hamster; /, from the blood of a squirrel. 394 BLOOD AND LYMPH. cases also as to the form that the crystals take. In man and in most of the mammalia hemoglobin is deposited in the form of rhombic prisms; in the guinea pig it crystallizes in tetrahedra (d, Fig. 168), and in the squirrel in hexagonal plates. This difference in crystal- line form implies some difference in molecular structure, and taken together with other known variations in property shown by hemo- globin from different animals lead us to believe that the huge mole- cule has a labile structure, and that it may differ somewhat in its molecular composition or atomic arrangement without losing its physiological property of an oxygen-carrier. In this connection it is interesting to state that the hemoglobin of horses' blood, which crystallizes ordinarily in large rhombic prisms, may be made to give hexagonal crystals by allowing it to undergo putrefaction, and that the form of the crystals may then be changed from hexagons to rhombs by varying the temperature of the solutions.* The crystals are readily soluble in water, and by repeated crystallization the hemoglobin may be obtained perfectly pure. As- in the case of other soluble proteid-like bodies, solutions of hemoglobin are precipitated by alcohol, by mineral acids, by salts of the heavy metals, by boiling, etc. Notwithstanding the fact that hemoglobin crystallizes so readily, it is not easily dialyzable, behaving in this respect like proteids and other colloidal bodies. The compounds which hemoglobin forms with carbon monoxid (CO) and nitric oxid (NO) are also crystallizable, the crystals being isomorphous with those of oxyhemoglobin. Absorption Spectra. — Solutions of hemoglobin and its deriv- ative compounds, when examined with a spectroscope, give distinctive absorption bands. Light, when made to pass through a glass prism, is broken up into its constituent rays, giving the play of rainbow colors known as the spectrum. A spectroscope is an apparatus for producing and observing a spectrum. A simple form, which illustrates sufficiently well the construction of the appara- tus, is shown in Fig. 169, P being the glass prism giving the spectrum. Light falls upon this prism through the tube (A) to the left, known as the "colli- mator tube." A slit at the end of this tube (S) admits a narrow slice of light — lamplight or sunlight — which then, by means of a convex lens at the other end of the tube, is made to fall upon the prism (P) with its rays parallel. In passing through the prism the rays are dispersed by unequal refraction, giving a spectrum. The spectrum thus produced is examined by the observer with the aid of the telescope (B). When the telescope is properly focused for the rays entering it from the prism (P), a clear picture of the spectrum is seen. The length of the spectrum will depend upon the nature and the number of the prisms through which the light is made to pass. For ordinary purposes a short spectrum is preferable for hemoglobin bands, and a spectroscope with one prism is generally used. If the source of light is a lamp flame of some kind, the spectrum is continuous, the colors gradually merging one into another from red to violet. If sunlight is used, the spectrum will be crossed by a number of narrow dark lines known as the "Fraunhofer lines." The position of these lines in the solar spectrum is fixed, and the more distinct ones are * Uhlik, "Archiv f. d. gesammte Physiologie," 104, 64, 1904. GENERAL PROPERTIES: THE CORPUSCLES. 395 designated by letters of the alphabet, A, B,C, D, E, etc., as shown in the charts below. If while using solar light or an artificial light a solution of any sub- stance which gives absorption bands is so placed in front of the slit that the light is obliged to traverse it, the spectrum as observed through the telescope will show one or more narrow or broad black bands that are characteristic of the substance used and constitute its absorption spectrum. The positions of these bands may be designated by describing their relations to the Fraun- hofer lines, or more directly by stating the wave lengths of the portions of the spectrum between which absorption takes place. Some spectroscopes are provided with a scale of wave lengths superposed on the spectrum, and when properly adjusted this scale enables one to read off directly the wave lengths of any part of the spectrum. When very dilute solutions of oxyhemoglobin are examined with the spectroscope, two absorption bands appear, both occurring in Fig. 169. — Spectroscope : P, The glass prism ; A, the collimator tube, showing the slit, S, through which the light is admitted; B, the telescope for observing the spectrum. the portion of the spectrum included between the Fraunhofer lines D and E. The band nearer the red end of the spectrum is known as the "a-band"; it is narrower, darker, and more clearly defined than the other, the "/3-band" (Fig. 170). With a solution con- taining 0.09 per cent, of oxyhemoglobin, and examined in layers one centimeter thick, the a-band extends over the part of the spectrum included between the wave-lengths X 583 (yrinnnnr °f a millimeter) and X 571, and the /?-band between X 550 and X 532 (Gamgee). The width and distinctness of the bands vary naturally with the concentration of the solution used (see Fig. 171), or, if the con- centration remains the same, with the width of the stratum of liquid through which the light passes. With a certain minimal percentage 396 BLOOD AND LYMPH. of oxyhemoglobin (less than 0.01 per cent.) the /5-band is lost and the a-band is very faint in layers one centimeter thick. With stronger solutions the bands become darker and wider and finally fuse, while some of the extreme red end and a great deal of the violet end of the spectrum is also absorbed. The variations in the absorp- tion spectrum, with differences in concentration, are clearly shown in the accompanying illustration from Rollett * (Fig. 171) ; the thick- 660 c r> e 650 6k0 630 620 610 600 sk) 580 570 560 550 5k0 53d\ 520 I.— 1....I....1....I ll.ltlll,Ji»lllmlltilll.Mlltnl|.|JtllllHMllllllLllllllllllllLl.llllllll.llllJl mo J Fig. 170. — Table of absorption spectra (Ziemke and Mutter) : 1, Absorption spectrum of oxyhemoglobin, dilute solution; 2, absorption spectrum of reduced hemoglobin; 3, ab- sorption spectrum of methemoglobin, neutral solution ; 4, absorption spectrum of met- hemoglobin, alkaline solution ; 5, absorption spectrum of hematin, acid solution ; 6, ab- sorption spectrum of hematin, alkaline solution. ness of the layer of liquid is supposed to be one centimeter. The numbers on the right indicate the percentage strength of the oxyhem- oglobin solutions. It will be noticed that the absorption which takes place as the concentration of the solution increases affects the red-orange end of the spectrum last of all. Solutions of reduced hemoglobin examined with the spectroscope * Hermann's "Handbuch der Physiologie," vol. iv, 1880. GENERAL PROPERTIES". THE CORPUSCLES. 397 show only one absorption band, known sometimes as the uf band." This band lies also in the portion of the spectrum included between the lines D and E; its relations to these lines and the bands of oxyhemoglobin are shown in Fig. 170. The r-band is much more diffuse than the oxyhemoglobin bands, and its limits, therefore, especially in weak solutions, are not well defined; in solutions of blood diluted 100 times with water, which would give a hemoglobin solution of about 0.14 per cent., the absorp- tion band lies in the part of the spectrum included between the wave lengths X 572 and X 542. The width and distinctness of this band vary also with the con- centration of the solu- tion. This variation is sufficiently well shown in the accompanying illustration (Fig. 172), which is a companion figure to the one just given for oxyhemo- globin (Fig. 171). It will be noticed that the last light to be absorbed in this case is partly in the red end and partly in the blue, thus ex- plaining the purplish color of hemoglobin so- lutions and of venous blood. Oxyhemoglobin solutions can be con- verted to hemoglobin solutions, with a corresponding change in the spectrum bands, by placing the former in a vacuum or, more conveniently, by adding reducing solutions. The solutions most commonly used for this purpose are ammonium sulphid and Stokes's reagent.* If a solu- tion of reduced hemoglobin is shaken with air, it quickly changes to aBC Fig. 171. — Diagram to show the variations in the absorption spectrum of oxyhemoglobin with varying concentrations of the solution. — (After Rollett.) The numbers to the right give the strength of the oxy- hemoglobin solution in percentages; the letters give the positions of the Fraunhofer lines. To ascertain the amount of absorption for any given concentration up to 1 per cent., draw a horizontal line across the diagram at the level corresponding to the concentra- tion. Where this line passes through the shaded part of the diagram absorption takes place, and the width of the absorption bands is seen at once. The diagram shows clearly that the amount of absorption increases as the solutions become more concentrated, especially the absorption of the blue end of the spectrum. It will be noticed that with concentrations between 0.6 and 0.7 per cent, the two bands between D and E fuse into one. * Stokes's reagent is an ammoniacal solution of a ferrous salt. It is made by dissolving 2 parts (by weight) of ferrous sulphate, adding 3 parts of tar- taric acid, and then ammonia to distinct alkaline reaction. A permanent precipitate should not be obtained. BLOOD AND LYMPH. emoglobin and gives two bands instead of one when exam- through the spectroscope. Any given solution may be changed is way from oxyhemoglobin to hemoglobin, and the reverse, a t number of times, thus demonstrating the facility with which moglobin takes up and surrenders oxygen. Solutions of carbon monoxid hemoglobin also give a spec- trum with two absorption bands closely resembling in posi- tion and appearance those of oxyhemoglobin. They are dis- tinguished from the oxyhemoglobin bands by being slightly nearer the blue end of the spectrum, as may be demonstrated by observing the wave lengths or, more con- veniently, by super- posing the two spectra. Moreover, solutions of carbon monoxid hem- oglobin are not re- duced to hemoglobin by adding Stokes's liquid, two bands be- ing still seen after such treatment. A solu- tion of carbon mon- oxid hemoglobin suit- able for spectroscopic examination may be prepared easily by passing ordinary coal- gas through a dilute oxyhemoglobin solution for a few minutes and then filtering. Derivative Compounds of Hemoglobin. — There are a number of pigmentary bodies which are formed directly from hemoglobin by decompositions or chemical reactions of various kinds. Some of these derivative substances occur normally in the body. The best known are as follows * : Methemoglobin. — When blood or a solution of oxyhemoglobin is allowed to stand for a long time exposed to the air it undergoes aBC Fig. 172. — Diagram to show the variations in the ab- sorption spectrum of reduced hemoglobin with vary- ing concentrations of the solution (after Rollett). The numbers to the right give the strength of the hemo- globin solution in percentages; the letters give the posi- tions of the Fraunhofer lines. For further directions as to the use of the diagram, see the description of Fig. 171. * For more detailed information concerning the chemistry and literature of these compounds see Hammarsten, "Physiological Chemistry," translated by Mandel, fourth edition, 1904; Cohnheim, "Chemie der Eiweisskorper," second edition, 1904. GENERAL PROPERTIES: THE CORPUSCLES. m a change in color, taking on a brownish tint. This change is du i to the formation of methemoglobin, and it is said that to some exl ent the transition occurs very soon after the blood is exposed to the iliV and that, therefore, determinations of the quantity of hemoglobin by the ordinary colorimetric methods should be made promptly W avoid a deterioration in color value. Methemoglobin may beS^ obtained rapidly by the action of various reagents on the blood, some of them oxidizing substances, such as permanganate of potash or ferricyanid of potash, some of them reducing substances. In- deed, it is known that the change may occur within the blood-vessels by the action of such bodies as the nitrites, antifebrin, acetanilid, etc. According to most observers, methemoglobin contains the same amount of oxygen as hemoglobin; it is combined differently, however, forming a more stable compound, which can not be dis- sociated by the action of a vacuum. On this account, therefore, methemoglobin is not capable of acting as a respiratory pigment, and to the extent that it is formed in the blood this tissue suffers a loss of its functional value as a carrier of oxygen. By the stronger action of reducing solutions — such as ammonium sulphid — the oxygen may be removed from the methemoglobin and reduced hemoglobin be obtained. Methemoglobin crystallizes in needles, and its solutions give an absorption spectrum which varies ac- cording as the solution is neutral or has an alkaline reaction. In neutral solutions the characteristic band is one in the orange, as indicated in Fig. 170. In alkaline solution the absorption spectrum has three bands, two of which are nearly identical with those of oxyhemoglobin. Hematin (C34H34N4Fe05) is obtained when hemoglobin is de- composed by the action of acids or alkalies in the presence or oxygen. It may occur in the feces if the diet contains hemoglobin or hematin, or in case of hemorrhages in the stomach or small intestine, since both the pancreatic and the gastric secretion break up hemoglobin, with the formation of hematin. It is an amorphous substance, of a dark-brown color, easily soluble in alkalies or in acid alcoholic solu- tions. These solutions give a characteristic absorption spectrum which is represented in Fig. 170. Hemin (C34H3304N4FeCl) is regarded as the hydrochloric acid ester of hematin and is obtained by the action of HC1 upon blood previously treated with alcohol. The compound is obtained in the form of crystals, which under the microscope appear usually as small, rhombic plates of a dark-brown color. These crystals may be obtained from small quantities of blood stains, etc., no matter how old, and they have been relied upon, therefore, as a sure and easy test for the existence of blood, — that is, hemoglobin. The test is one that has been much used in medicolegal cases, and may 400 BLOOD AND LYMPH. be carried out as follows: A bit of dried blood is powdered with a few crystals of NaCl. Some of the powder is placed upon a glass slide and covered with a cover-slip. By means of a pipette a drop or two of glacial acetic acid is run under the slip, and then by draw- ing the slide repeatedly through a flame the acid is evaporated to dryness, taking care not to heat the acid so high as to cause it to boil. After the evaporation of the acid water is run under the slip and the specimen is ready for examination with the microscope. Hemochromogen (C 3 4H36N4FeO 5 ?) is obtained when hemoglobin is decomposed by acids or alkalies in the absence of free oxygen. By oxidation it is converted to hematin. Hemochromogen is crystal- line, and gives a characteristic absorption spectrum. Hematoporphyrin (C34H38N406) differs from the preceding deriv- atives of hemoglobin in that it contains no iron. It may be ob- tained from hematin by the action of strong acids, and is of much physiological interest because of its relationship to the bile pigments, which, like it, are iron-free derivatives of the hemoglobin. In old blood-clots or extravasations it has long been known that a colored crystalline product may be formed. This product was designated as hematoidin by Virchow and later was stated, on the one hand, to be identical with the bile pigment, bilirubin, and, on the other hand, to be isomeric with hematoporphyrin. Later observers have prepared from hematoporphyrin by careful reduction a substance designated as mesoporphyrin. It contains one less oxygen atom than the hematoporphyrin, and is claimed to be identical with hematoidin. Another fact of great general interest is that from plant chlorophyl there may be prepared a compound, phylloporphy- rin, very similar to the mesoporphyrin. It would appear from this relationship that the red coloring matter of the blood and the green coloring matter of plants are compounds that have some similarity in chemical structure. Histohematins. — This name is a general term that has been given to the coloring matter found in the tissues, so far as it has the property of taking up oxygen. The red coloring matter in some muscles is an example of such a compound and has been designated specifically as myohematin. According to most observers, myo- hematin is identical with hemoglobin, — that is, the muscle substance contains some hemoglobin, — and we may suppose that its presence in the tissue furnishes a further means for the transportation of oxygen to the muscle protoplasm. Bile Pigments and Urinary Pigments. — Both of these pigments are referred to in the description of the composition of bile and urine. In this connection the fact may be emphasized that each of them is supposed to be derived from hemoglobin, and each constitutes, so to speak, a form of excretion of hemoglobin. 401 ^ag^J^it cal Ucp' Origin and Fate of the Red Corpuscles. — The mammalian red corpuscle is a cell that has lost its nucleus. It is not probable, there- fore, that any given corpuscle lives for a great while in the circulation. This is made more certain by the fact that hemoglobin is the mother substance from which the bile pigments are made, and, as these pigments are being excreted continually, it is fair to suppose that red corpuscles are as steadily undergoing disintegration in the blood- stream. Just how long the average life of the corpuscles may be has not been determined, nor is it certain where and how they go to pieces. It has been suggested that their destruction takes place in the spleen, but the observations advanced in support of this hypothesis are not very numerous or conclusive. Among the rea- sons given for assuming that the spleen is especially concerned in the destruction of red corpuscles, the most weighty is the histo- logical fact that one can sometimes find in teased preparations oi spleen-tissue certain large cells which contain red corpuscles in their cell-substance in various stages of disintegration. It has been supposed that the large cells actually ingest the red corpuscles, selecting those, presumably, that are in a state of physiological de- cline. Against this idea a number of objections may be raised. Large leucocytes with red corpuscles in their interior are not found so frequently nor so constantly in the spleen as we should expect would be the case if the act of ingestion were constantly going on. There is some reason for believing, indeed, that the whole act of ingestion may be a postmortem phenomenon; that is, after the cessation of the blood-stream the ameboid movements of the large leucocytes continue, while the red corpuscles lie at rest, — conditions that are favorable to the act of ingestion. It may be added also that the blood of the splenic vein contains no hemoglobin in solu- tion, indicating that no considerable dissolution of red corpuscles is taking place in the spleen. Moreover, complete extirpation of the spleen does not seem to lessen materially the normal destruction of red corpuscles, if we may measure the extent of that normal destruction by the quantity of bile pigment formed in the liver, remembering that hemoglobin is the mother-substance from which the bile pigments are derived. It is more probable that there is no special organ or tissue charged with the function of destroying red corpuscles, but that they undergo disintegration and dissolution while in the blood-stream and in any part of the circulation, the liberated hemoglobin being carried to the liver and excreted in part as bile pigment. The continual destruction of red corpuscles implies, of course, a continual formation of new ones. It has been shown satisfactorily that in the adult the organ for the reproduction of red corpuscles is the red marrow of bones. In this tissue hema- topoiesis, as the process of formation of red corpuscles is termed, goe? 26 402 BLOOD AND LYMPH. on continually, the process being much increased after hemorrhages and in certain pathological conditions. The details of the histo- logical changes will be found in the text-books of histology. It is sufficient here to state simply that a group of nucleated, colorless cells, erythroblasts, is found in the red marrow. These cells multiply by karyokinesis, and the daughter-cells eventually produce hemoglobin in their cytoplasm, thus forming nucleated red cor- puscles or normoblasts. The nuclei are subsequently lost, either by disintegration or by extrusion, and the newly formed non-nucleated red corpuscles (erythrocytes) are forced into the blood-stream, owing to a gradual change in their position during development caused by the growing hematopoietic tissue. When the process is greatly accelerated, as after severe hemorrhages or in certain pathological conditions, red corpuscles still retaining their nuclei (normoblasts) may be found in the circulating blood, having been forced out prematurely. Such corpuscles may subsequently lose their nuclei while in the blood-stream. In the embryo, hema- topoietic tissue is found in parts of the body other than the marrow, notably in the liver and spleen, which at that time serve as organs for the production of new red corpuscles. In the blood of the young embryo nucleated red corpuscles are at first abundant, but they become less numerous as the fetus grows older.* Variations in the Number of Red Corpuscles. — The average number of red corpuscles for the adult male, as has been stated already, is usually given as 5,000,000 per c.mm. The number is found to vary greatly, however. Outside pathological con- ditions, in which the diminution in number may be extreme, dif- ferences have been observed in human beings under such conditions as the following : The number is less in females (4,500,000) ; it varies in individuals with the constitution, nutrition, and manner of life; it varies with age, being greatest in the fetus and in the new-born child; it varies with the time of the day, showing a distinct diminu- tion after meals; in the female it varies somewhat in menstruation and in pregnancy, being slightly increased in the former and di- minished in the latter condition. Variation with Altitude. — Perhaps the most interesting of the conditions that may influence the number of the blood corpuscles is a change in altitude. Attention was first directed to this point by Bert,f who believed that the diminished supply of oxygen in high altitudes may be compensated by an increased amount of hemoglobin, and subsequently ViaultJ demonstrated that living for * Howell: "Life History of the Blood Corpuscles," etc., "Journal of Morphology," 1890, vol. iv. f Bert, "La pression barometrique," 1878, p. 1108. J Viault, "Comptes rendus de l'academie des sciences," 1890 and 1891. GENERAL PROPERTIES! THE CORPUSCLES. 403 a short time at very high altitudes (4000 meters) causes a marked in- crease in the number of red corpuscles, — an increase, for instance, from 5,000,000 per c.mm. to 7,000,000 or even 8,000,000. This fact has since been investigated with great care by a large number of observers and under a great variety of conditions. The observation has been abundantly confirmed, and indeed it would seem that the reaction takes place very quickly. Within twenty-four hours, according to some observers, and in less time, according to others who have experimented during balloon ascensions (Gaule, Hallion, and Tissot), the increase in the number of corpuscles may be de- tected, although the maximum increase comes on more gradually. According to Kemp,* the number of blood plates is also greatly increased by high altitudes, while the leucocytes are not affected. There has, however, been much difference of opinion as to whether this increase in number of the red corpuscles is relative or absolute, — that is, whether the total number of red corpuscles in the blood, and therefore probably the total amount of hemoglobin, is increased, or whether it is simply an apparent increase due, for instance, to a diminution in the water of the blood and a consequent concentration as regards the number of corpuscles. The results published upon these questions are so conflicting that it is difficult to make any positive statements at present. One may, however, believe that the increased number or concentration of red corpuscles is an adap- tation by means of which the oxygen-carrying capacity of the blood is raised to compensate for the diminished amount of oxygen in the air. According to one set of observers, this adaptation is brought about by an actual — that is, an absolute — increase in the total number of red corpuscles, and therefore in the total amount of hemoglobin. According to this explanation, one must assume fhat the diminished amount of oxygen in the air at high altitudes or some other condition peculiar to these altitudes acts as a stimulus to the blood-forming tissues (red marrow) and augments the output of corpuscles. According to another set of observers, the adap- tation is brought about by a concentration of the blood. The blood- plasma is reduced in quantity, perhaps by transudation of water into the tissues, and therefore the number of red corpuscles and the amount of hemoglobin become greater for each cubic millimeter. If we assume that this smaller bulk of blood, more concentrated in corpuscles and hemoglobin, circulates more rapidly, then also the oxygen-carrying capacity of the blood is increased. Recent evidence seem to favor this latter explanation, particularly as regards the nearly immediate effects of a change in altitude. Abderhalden, for instance, has shown that, if animals of the same species and same litter are bled to death and the total * Kemp, "American Journal of Physiology," 10, 34, 1904. 404 BLOOD AND LYMPH. quantity of hemoglobin is estimated, the average figures obtained for the animals at low levels are the same as for those at the high altitudes.* Physiology of the Blood Leucocytes. — The function of the blood leucocytes has been the subject of numerous investigations, particularly in connection with the pathology of blood diseases. Although many hypotheses have been made as the result of this work, it can not be said that we possess any positive information as to the normal function of these cells in the body. It must be borne in mind, in the first place, that the blood leucocytes are not all the same histologically, and it may be that their functions are as diverse as their morphology. Various classifications have been made, based upon one or another difference in microscopical structure and reaction, but at present the system used by most authors is that adopted by Ehrlich.f According to this nomenclature, the white corpuscles fall into two main groups, — the lymphocytes and the leucocytes, — and each of these into two or more subgroups. Thus : I. Lymphocytes. No granules in the cell substance, and, though capable of ameboid changes of form, this property is not characteristic and prob- ably not sufficient to cause locomotion. (a) Small lymphocytes are about the size of the red corpuscles; the nu- cleus is large, symmetrically placed, stains homogeneously, and the cytoplasm is reduced to a very small amount. They form from 20 to 25 per cent, of all the white corpuscles. (b) Large lymphocytes. Two to three times as large as the preceding. Nucleus somewhat eccentric; the cytoplasm is relatively more abundant than in a, but non-granular. These forms exist only in small numbers, forming 1 per cent, or less of the white corpuscles. II, Leucocytes. Granules of different sorts found in the cytoplasm. Cells characteristically ameboid, (a) Transition forms (uninuclear leucocytes). Single large nucleus, more or less lobulated; cytoplasm abundant and faintly granulated. The granules stain with neutral dyes and are therefore designated as neutrophile granules. The name, transition form, implies that these leucocytes represent an intermediate stage between the large lympho- cytes and the following variety. This form exists in small numbers, — 2 to 10 per cent, of the total number of white corpuscles. (6) Polynuclear or polymorphonuclear leucocytes. The nucleus is seg- mented into lobes connected by narrow strands. The cytoplasm is especially ameboid and is granular. The granules in most cases are neutrophilic and small in size. The typical cells of this kind form the bulk of the white corpuscles of the blood, — 60 to 75 per cent. Eosinophilic leucocytes form a subgroup of this variety. They have a similar segmented nucleus, but the cytoplasm contains nu- merous coarse granules that stain in acid dyes, such as eosin, whence the name. (c) Mast cells. These peculiar cells exist in very small numbers under normal conditions, — less than 1 per cent, of the total number of * For the extensive literature see Van Voornveld, "Das Blut im Hoch- gebirge," "Pfliiger's Archiv," 92, 1, 1902; also Oliver, "A Contribution to the Study of Blood and Blood Pressure," London, 1901. t Ehrlich, "Die Anaemie," 1898; see also Seemann, "Ergebnisse der Physiologic," 3, part i, 1904. GENERAL PROPERTIES: THE CORPUSCLES. 405 white corpuscles. They have a polymorphic nucleus like the pre- ceding group, but differ in the fact that the granules in the cyto- plasm are strongly basophilic, — that is, will stain only with basic dyes, such as thionin. According to most of the authors who have studied the ap- pearance and variation in number of these cells under pathological conditions, the small lymphocytes are cells that have an origin and function different from those of the granular leucocytes. While the latter are supposed to originate from cells (leucoblasts, my- elocytes) in the bone marrow, the lymphocytes are produced in the nodules of the lymph glands and lymphoid tissue, and enter the blood through the lymph circulation. Another view maintained by some authors is that the various white corpuscles in the blood represent different stages of development. According to this view, the small lymphocytes as received from the lymph circulation are the young or immature form, and they develop, while in the circu- lation, through the stages represented by the large lymphocytes and the mononuclear leucocytes to the mature form, the poly- morphonuclear leucocytes. Variations in Number. — Under normal conditions the total number of leucocytes may show considerable variation; the aver- age number in health varies usually between 5000 and 7000 per cubic millimeter. A distinct increase in number is designated as a condition of leucocytosis, a marked diminution as a condition of leucopenia. Leucocytosis occurs under various normal conditions, such as digestion, exercise or cold baths, pregnancy, etc. The variations, relative or absolute, under pathological conditions, have been studied with exhaustive care as an aid to diagnosis and classi- fication. Functions of the Leucocytes. — Perhaps the most striking property of the leucocytes as a class is their power of making ameboid movements, — a characteristic which has gained for them the sobriquet of "wandering" cells. By virtue of this property some of them are able to migrate through the walls of blood capil- laries into the surrounding tissues. This process of migration takes place normally, but is vastly accelerated under pathological con- ditions. As to the function or functions fulfilled by the leucocytes, numerous suggestions have been made, some of which may be stated in brief form as follows: (1) They protect the body from pathogenic bacteria. In explanation of this action it has been suggested that they may either ingest the bacteria, and thus destroy them directly, or they may form certain substances, defensive proteids, that destroy the bacteria. Leucocytes that act by ingest- ing the bacteria are spoken of as a phagocytes" {r L 2 t 7 X-7 ^ iv 2 ^ t l^^N. "^ ^^ I t 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 „ [ 100 Erythrocyte*. Haemoglobin. Leucocyte* Fig. 173. — To show the effect of hemorrhage upon the number of red and white cor- puscles and the amount of hemoglobin. — (Dawson.) The ordinates express the numbers of corpuscles and also the percentages of hemoglobin as stated in the figures to the left. The abscissas give the days after hemorrhage. The experiment was made upon a dog of 8.1 kgms. The hemorrhage, which lasted 2.3 minutes, was equal to 4.3 per cent, of the body-weight. An equal amount of physiological saline (NaCl, 0.8 per cent.) was injected immediately. ume within a few hours in slight hemorrhages, and in from twenty- four to forty-eight hours if the loss of blood has been severe; but the number of red corpuscles and the hemoglobin are regenerated more slowly, getting back to normal only after a number of days or after several weeks. The accompanying curves illustrate the results of a severe hemorrhage (4.3 per cent, of the body- weight) followed by transfusion of an equal volume of physiological saline. So far as the red corpuscles and the amount of hemoglobin are concerned, it will be noticed that the large sudden fall from the hemorrhage, first day, is followed by a slower drop in both factors during the IL 426 BLOOD AND LYMPH. secc ad and third days. This latter phenomenon constitutes what 10 wn as the posthemorrhagic fall.* lood-transfusion. — Shortly after the discovery of the circu- ron of the blood (Harvey, 1628), the operation was introduced transfusing blood from one individual to another or from some «%*"# the lower animals to man. Extravagant hopes were held as to the value of such transfusion not only as a means of replacing the blood lost by hemorrhage, but also as a cure for various infirmities and diseases. Then and subsequently fatal as well as successful results followed the operation. It is now known to be a dangerous undertaking, mainly for two reasons: first, the strange blood, whether transfused directly or after defibrination, is liable to contain a quantity of fibrin ferment sufficient, perhaps, to cause intravas- cular clotting ; secondly, the serum of one animal may be toxic to another or cause a destruction of its blood corpuscles. Owing to this hemolytic and toxic action, which has previously been referred to (p. 387), the injection of foreign blood is likely to be directly injurious instead of beneficial. In cases of loss of blood from severe hemorrhage, therefore, it is far safer to inject a neutral liquid, such as the so-called " physiological salt solution" — a solution of sodium chlorid of such a strength (0.7 to 0.9) as will suffice to pre- vent hemolysis of the red corpuscles. * Dawson, "American Journal of Physiology," 4, 1, 1900. CHAPTER XXIV. COMPOSITION AND FORMATION OF LYMPH. Lymph is a colorless liquid found in the lymph-vessels as well as in the extra vascular spaces of the body. All the tissue elements, in fact, may be regarded as being bathed in lymph. To understand its occurrence in the body one has only to bear in mind its method of origin from the blood. Throughout the entire body there is a rich supply of blood-vessels penetrating every tissue with the ex- ception of the epidermis and some epidermal structures, as the nails and the hair. The plasma of the blood, by the action of physical or chemical processes, the details of which are not yet entirely under- stood, makes its way through the thin walls of the capillaries, and is thus brought into immediate contact with the tissues, to which it brings the nourishment and oxygen of the blood and from which it removes the waste products of metabolism. This extravascular lymph is collected into small capillary spaces that in turn open into definite lymphatic vessels. It is still a question among the his- tologists whether the lymph- vessels form a closed system or are in direct anatomical connection with the tissue spaces. Recent work* supports the view that the lymph capillaries are closed vessels simi- lar in structure to the blood capillaries. They end in the tissues generally, but are not in open communication with the spaces between the cellular elements or with the larger serous cavities between the folds of the peritoneum, pleura, etc. From the phys- iological standpoint, however, the liquid in these latter cavities, the cerebrospinal liquid, and the liquid bathing the tissue elements must be regarded as a part of the general supply of lymph and as being in communication with the liquid contained in the lymph- vessels. That is to say, the water and the dissolved substances contained in the tissue spaces interchange more or less freely with the lymph proper found in the formed lymph- vessels. The lymph- vessels unite to form larger and larger trunks, making eventually one main trunk, the thoracic or left lymphatic duct, and a second smaller right lymphatic duct, which open into the blood-vessels, each on its own side, at the junction of the subclavian and internal jugular veins. While the supply of lymph in the lymph- vessels may * See MaCallum, "Bulletin of the Johns Hopkins Hospital," 14, 1, 1903; also Sabin, " American Journal of Anatomy," 1, 367, 1902, and 3, 183, 1904. 427 g2 tr ^> X ', I 9 — • 428 BLOOD AND LYMPH. be considered as being derived ultimately entirely from the blood- plasma, it is well to bear in mind that at any given moment this supply may be altered by direct interchange with the plasma on one side and the extra vascular lymph permeating the tissue elements on the other. The intravascular lymph may be augmented, for ex- ample, by a flow of water from the plasma into the lymph spaces, or by a flow from the tissue elements into the lymph spaces that surround them. The lymph movement is from the tissues to the veins, and the flow is maintained chiefly by the difference in pres- sure between the lymph at its origin in the tissues and in the large lymphatic vessels. The continual formation of lymph in the tissues leads to the development of a relatively high pressure in the lymph capillaries, and as a result of this the lymph is forced toward the point of lowest pressure, — namely, the points of junction of the large lymph ducts with the venous system. A fuller discussion of the factors concerned in the movement of lymph will be found in the section on circulation. As would be inferred from its origin, the composition of lymph is essentially the same as that of blood- plasma. It contains the three blood proteids, the extractives (urea, fat, lecithin, cholesterin, sugar), and inorganic salts. The salts are found in the same proportions as in the plasma ; the proteids are less in amount, especially the fibrinogen. Lymph coagulates, but does so more slowly and less firmly than the blood. Histologically , lymph consists of a colorless liquid containing a number of leuco- cytes, and after meals a number of minute fat droplets; red blood corpuscles occur only accidentally, and blood plates, according to most accounts, are likewise normally absent. Formation of Lymph. — The careful researches of Ludwig and his pupils were formerly believed to prove that the lymph is derived directly from the plasma of the blood mainly by filtration through the capillary walls. Emphasis was laid on the undoubted fact that the blood within the capillaries is under a pressure higher than that prevailing in the tissues outside, and it was supposed that this excess of pressure is sufficient to squeeze the plasma of the blood through the very thin capillary walls. Various conditions that alter the pressure of the blood were shown to influence the amount of lymph formed in accordance with the demands of a theory of filtration. Moreover, the composition of lymph as usually given seems to sup- port such a theory, inasmuch as the inorganic salts contained in it are in the same concentration, approximately, as in blood-plasma, while the proteids are in less concentration, following the well- known law that in the filtration of colloids through animal mem- branes the filtrate is more dilute than the original solution. This simple and apparently satisfactory theory has been subjected to critical examination within recent years, and it has been shown that COMPOSITION AND FORMATION OF LYMPH. 429 filtration alone does not suffice to explain the composition of the lymph under all circumstances. At present two divergent views are held upon the subject. According to some physiologists, all the facts known with regard to the composition of lymph may be satisfactorily explained if we suppose that this liquid is formed from blood-plasma by the combined action of the physical processes of filtration, diffusion, and osmosis. According to others, it is believed that, in addition to filtration and diffusion, it is necessary to assume an active secretory process on the part of the endothe- lial cells composing the capillary walls. The actual condition of our knowledge of the subject can be presented most easily by briefly stating some of the objections that have been raised by Heidenhain* to a pure filtration-and-diffusion theory, and indicating how these objections have been met. 1. Heidenhain shows by simple calculations that an impossible formation of lymph would be required, upon the filtration theory, to supply the chemical needs of the organs in various organic and in- organic constituents. Thus, to take an illustration that has been much discussed, one kilogram of cows' milk contains 1.7 gms. CaO and the entire milk of twenty-four hours would contain, in round numbers, 42.5 gms. CaO. Since the lymph contains normally about 0.18 part of CaO per thousand, it would require 236 liters of lymph per day to supply the necessary CaO to the mammary glands. Heidenhain himself suggests that the difficulty in this case may be met by assuming active diffusion processes in connection with filtration. If, for instance, in the case cited, we suppose that the CaO of the lymph is quickly combined by the tissues of the mam- mary gland, then the tension of calcium salts in the lymph will be kept at zero, and an active diffusion of calcium into the lymph will occur so long as the gland is secreting. In other words, the gland will receive its calcium by much the same process as it receives its oxygen, and will get its daily supply from a comparatively small bulk of lymph. Strictly speaking, therefore, the difficulty we are dealing with here shows only the insufficiency of a pure filtration theory. It seems possible that filtration and diffusion together would suffice to supply the organs, so far at least as the diffusible substances are concerned. 2. Heidenhain found that occlusion of the inferior vena cava causes not only an increase in the flow of lymph — as might be ex- pected, on the filtration theory, from the consequent rise of pressure in the capillary regions — but also an increased concentration in the percentage of proteid in the lymph. This latter fact has been satisfactorily explained by the experiments of Starling.f Accord- * "Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie," 49, 209, 1891. t "Journal of Physiology," 16, 234, 1894. 430 BLOOD AND LYMPH. ing to this observer, the lymph formed in the liver is normally more concentrated than that of the rest of the body. The occlusion of the vena cava causes a marked rise in the capillary pressure in the liver, and most of the increased lymph-flow under these circum- stances comes from the liver; hence the greater concentration. The results of this experiment, therefore, do not antagonize the filtration-and-diffusion theory. 3. Heidenhain discovered that extracts of various substances, which he designated as " lymphagogues of the first class," cause a marked increase in the flow of lymph from the thoracic duct, the lymph being more concentrated than normal, and the increased flow continuing for a long period. Nevertheless, these substances cause little, if any, increase in general arterial pressure; in fact, if injected in sufficient quantity they produce usually a fall of arterial pressure. The substances belonging to this class comprise such things as pep- tone, egg-albumin, extracts of liver and intestine, and especially extracts of the muscles of crabs, crayfish, mussels, and leeches. Heidenhain supposed that these extracts contain an organic substance which acts as a specific stimulus to the endothelial cells of the capillaries and increases their secretory action. The results of the action of these substances has been differently explained by those who are unwilling to believe in the secretion theory. Starling * finds experimentally that the increased flow of lymph in this case, as after obstruction of the vena cava, comes mainly from the liver. There is at the same time in the portal area an increased pressure that may account in part for the greater flow of lymph; but, since this effect upon the portal pressure lasts but a short time, while the greater flow of lymph may continue for one or two hours, it is obvious that this factor alone does not suffice to explain the result of the injections. Starling suggests, therefore, that these extracts act pathologically upon the blood capillaries, particularly those of the liver, and render them more permeable, so that a greater quantity of concentrated lymph flows through them. Starling's explanation is supported by the experiments of Popoff .f According to this observer, if the lymph is collected simultaneously from the lower portion of the thoracic duct, which conveys the lymph from the abdominal organs, and from the upper part, which contains the lymph from the head, neck, etc., it is found that injection of peptone increases the flow only from the abdominal organs. Popoff finds also that the peptone causes a dilatation in the intestinal circulation and a marked rise in the portal pressure. At the same time there is some evidence of injury to the walls of the blood- vessels from the occurrence of extravasations in the intestine. As * "Journal of Physiology," 17, 30, 1894. f "Centralblatt f. Physiologie," 9, No. 2, 1895. COMPOSITION AND FORMATION OF LYMPH. 431 far, therefore, as the action of the lymphagogues of the first class is concerned, it may- be said that the advocates of the filtration-and- diffusion theory have suggested a plausible explanation in accord with their theory. The facts emphasized by Heidenhain with regard to this class of substances do not compel us to assume a secretory function for the endothelial cells. 4. Injection of certain crystalline substances — such as sugar, sodium chlorid, and other neutral salts — causes a marked increase in the flow of lymph from the thoracic duct. The lymph in these cases is more dilute than normal, and the blood-plasma also becomes more watery, thus indicating that the increase in water comes from the tissues themselves. Heidenhain designated these bodies as "lymphagogues of the second class." His explanation of their action is that the crystalloid materials introduced into the blood are eliminated by the secretory activity of the endothelial cells, and that they then attract water from the tissue elements, thus augmenting the flow of lymph. These substances cause but little change in arterial blood-pressure; hence Heidenhain thought that the greater flow of lymph can not be explained by an increased filtration. Starling * has shown, however, that, although these bodies may not seriously alter general arterial pressure, they may greatly augment intracapillary pressure, particularly in the abdominal organs. His explanation of the greater flow of lymph in these cases is as follows : " On their injection into the blood the osmotic pressure of the circu- lating fluid is largely increased. In consequence of this increase water is attracted from lymph and tissues into the blood by a process of osmosis, until the osmotic pressure of the circulating fluid is restored to normal. A condition of hydremic plethora is thereby produced, attended with a rise of pressure in the capillaries generally, especially in those of the abdominal viscera. This rise of pressure will be proportional to the increase in the volume of the blood, and therefore to the osmotic pressure of the solutions injected. The rise of capillary pressure causes great increase in the transudation of fluid from the capillaries, and therefore in the lymph-flow from the thoracic duct." This explanation is well supported by experi- ments, and seems to obviate the necessity of assuming a secretory action on the part of the capillary walls. 5. Numerous other experiments have been devised by Heidenhain and his followers to show that the physical laws of filtration, diffu- sion, and osmosis do not suffice to explain the movement of lymph; but in all cases possible explanations have been suggested in terms of the physical laws, so that it may be said that the facts do not compel us to assume a secretory activity on the part of the endo- thelial cells of the capillaries. In recent years Asherf and his * hoc. cit. t "Zeitschrift f. Biologie," vols, xxxvi-xl, 1897 to 1900. 432 BLOOD AND LYMPH. co-workers have brought forward many facts to show that the lymph is controlled as to its amount by the activity of the tissue elements and may be considered as a product of the activity of the tissues, as a secretion, in fact, of the working cells. When the salivary glands, the liver, etc., are in greater functional activity the flow of lymph from them is increased beyond doubt, so that the activity of the organs does influence most markedly the production of lymph. Most physiologists, however, prefer to explain this relationship on the view suggested by Koranyi, Starling, and others, — namely, that in the metabolic changes of functional activity the large molecules of proteid, fat, etc., are broken down to a number of simpler ones, the number of particles in solution is increased and therefore the osmotic pressure is increased. By this means it may be supposed that the flow of lymph toward the tissue elements is increased in proportion to their activity. The lymph in the tissue spaces between the cells is subjected to many influences which, taken together, regulate its amount. It is continually augmented by a flow of water and dissolved substances from the blood in the capillaries and from the liquid in the interior of the cells, and it is continually depleted by the excess passing off into the lymphatics, on the one hand, through which it eventually reaches the blood, and also by direct absorption into the blood capillaries. In regard to this last factor, there is abundant evidence that solutions injected into the tissue spaces so as to increase the amount or concentration of the tissue liquid are promptly absorbed into the blood. The play of these opposing forces maintains the tissue lymph within normal limics. and, although the movement of the water and dissolved sub stances can not be shown in all cases to be governed solely by the physical processes of diffusion, osmosis, and filtration, there is at present no conclusive evidence that these factors are in- sufficient to account for the regulation. Summary of the Factors Controlling the Flow of Lymph. — We may adopt, provisionally at least, the so-called mechanical theory of the origin of lymph. Upon this theory the forces in activity are, first, the intracapillary pressure tending to filter the plasma through the endothelial cells composing the walls of the capillaries; second, the force of diffusion depending upon the inequality in chemical composition of the blood-plasma and the liquid outside the capil- laries, or, on the other side, between this liquid and the contents of the tissue elements; third, the force of osmotic pressure. These three forces acting everywhere control primarily the amount and composition of the lymph; but still another factor must be con- sidered ; for when we come to examine the flow of lymph in different parts of the body striking differences are found. It has been shown, COMPOSITION AND FORMATION OF LYMPH. 433 for instance, that in the limbs, under normal conditions, the flow is extremely scanty, while from the liver and the intestinal area it is relatively abundant. In fact, the lymph of the thoracic duct may be considered as being derived almost entirely from the latter two regions. Moreover, the lymph from the liver is characterized by a greater percentage of proteids. To account for these differences Starling suggests the plausible explanation of a variation in permea- bility in the capillary walls. The capillaries seem to have a similar structure all over the body so far as this is revealed to us by the microscope, but the fact that the lymph-flow varies so much in quantity and composition indicates that the similarity is only superficial, and that in different organs the capillary walls may have different internal structures, and therefore different permea- bilities. This factor is evidently one of great importance. The idea that the permeability of the capillaries may vary under dif- ferent conditions has long been used in pathology to explain the production of that excess of lymph which gives rise to the condition of dropsy or edema. The theories and experiments made in con- nection with this pathological condition have, in fact, a direct bearing upon the theories of lymph formation.* Under normal conditions the lymph is drained off as it is formed, while under pathological conditions it may accumulate in the tissues owing either to an excessive formation of lymph or to some interruption in its circu- lation. From the foregoing considerations it is evident that changes in capillary pressure, however produced, may alter the flow of lymph from the blood-vessels to the tissues, by increasing or decreasing, as the case may be, the amount of filtration; changes in the composition of the blood, such as follow periods of digestion, will cause diffusion and osmotic streams tending to equalize the composition of blood and lymph; and changes in the tissues them- selves following upon physiological or pathological activity will also disturb the equilibrium of composition, and, therefore, set up diffusion and osmotic currents. In this way a continual interchange is taking place by means of which the nutrition of the tissues is effected, each according to its needs. The details of this interchange must of necessity be very complex when we consider the possibilities of local effects in different parts of the body. The total effects of general changes, such as may be produced experimentally, are simpler, and, as we have seen, are explained satisfactorily by the physical and chemical factors enumerated. ♦Consult Meltzer, "Edema" ("Harrington Lectures"), "American Medicine," 8, Nos. 1, 2, 4, and 5, 1904. 28 SECTION V. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE ORGANS OF CIRCULA- TION OF THE BLOOD AND LYMPH. The heart and the blood-vessels form a closed vascular system containing a certain amount of blood. This blood is kept in endless circLilation mainly by the force of the muscular contractions of the heart. But the bed through which it flows varies greatly in width at different parts of the circuit, and the resistance offered to the moving blood is very much greater in the capillaries than in the large vessels. It follows from the irregularities in size of the chan- nels through which it flows that the blood-stream is not uniform in character throughout the entire circuit; indeed, just the opposite is true. From point to point in the branching system of vessels the blood varies in regard to its velocity, its head of pressure, etc. These variations are connected in part with the fixed structure of the system and in part are dependent upon the changing properties of the living matter of which the system is composed. It is con- venient to consider the subject under three general heads: (1) The purely physical factors of the circulation, — that is, the me- chanics and hydrodynamics of the flow of a definite quantity of blood through a set of fixed tubes of varying caliber under certain fixed conditions. (2) The general physiology of the heart and the blood-vessels, — that is, mainly the special properties of the heart muscle and the plain muscles of the blood-vessels. (3) The innerva- tion of the heart and the blood-vessels, — that is, the variations in them produced by the action of the nervous system. CHAPTER XXV. THE VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF THE BLOOD-FLOW. The Circulation as Seen Under the Microscope. — It is a comparatively easy matter to arrange a thin membrane in a living animal so that the flowing blood may be observed with the aid of a microscope. For such a purpose one generally employs the web between the toes of a frog, or better still the mesentery, lungs, or 434 VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 435 bladder of the same animal. With a good preparation many important peculiarities in the blood-flow may be observed directly. If the field is properly chosen one may see at the same time the flow in arteries, capillaries, and veins. It will be noticed that in the arteries the flow is very rapid and somewhat intermittent, — that is, there is a slight acceleration of velocity, a pulse, with each heart beat. In the capillaries, on the contrary, the flow is relatively very slow; the change from the rushing arterial stream to the deliber- ate current in the capillaries takes place, indeed, with some suddenness. The capillary flow, as a rule, shows no pulses corre- sponding with the heart beats, but it may be more or less irregular, — that is, the flow may nearly cease at times in some capillaries, while again it maintains a constant flow. In the veins the flow increases markedly in rapidity, and indeed it may be observed that, the larger the vein, the more rapid is the flow. There is not, however, as a rule, any indication of an intermittence or pulse in this flow, — the velocity is entirely uniform. In both arteries and veins it will be noticed that the red corpuscles form a solid column or core in the middle of the vessel, and that between them and the inner wall there is a layer of plasma containing only, under normal conditions, an occasional leucocyte. The accumulation of cor- puscles in the middle of the stream makes what is known as the axial stream, while the clear layer of plasma is designated as the inert layer. The phenomenon is readily explained by physical causes. As the blood flows rapidly through the small vessels the layers nearer the wall are slowed by adhesion, so that the greatest velocity is attained in the middle or axis of the vessel. The cor- puscles, being heavier than the plasma, are drawn into this rapid part of the current. It has been shown by physical experiments that, when particles of different specific gravities are present in a liquid flowing rapidly through tubes, the heavier particles will be found in the axis and the lighter ones toward the periphery. In accordance with this fact, leucocytes, which are lighter than the red corpuscles, may be found in the inert layer. When the con- ditions become slightly abnormal (incipient inflammation) the leucocytes increase in number in the inert layer sometimes to a very great extent, owing apparently to some alteration in the endothelial walls whereby the leucocytes are rendered more ad- hesive. The agglutination of the leucocytes and their migration through the walls into the surrounding tissues can not be considered in this connection. The Velocity of the Blood-flow. — The microscopical observa- tions described above show that the velocity of the blood-current varies widely, being rapid in the arteries and veins and slow in the capillaries. To ascertain the actual velocity in the larger vessels 436 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. and the variations in vessels of different sizes experimental de- terminations are necessary. While the general principle involved in these determinations is simple, their actual execution in an experiment is attended with some difficulties, and various devices have been adopted. The most direct method per- haps is that used in the in- strument devised by Ludwig, — namely, the stromuhr. The prin- ciple used is to cut an artery or vein of a known size and de- termine how much blood flows out in a given time. We may define the velocity of the blood at any point as the length of the column of blood flowing by that point in a second. If we cut the artery there a cylindri- cal column of blood of a defi- nite length and with a cross-area equal to that of the lumen of the artery will flow out in a second. The volume of the outflow can be -determined di- rectly by catching the blood. Knowing this volume and the cross-area of the artery, we can determine the length of the column — that is, the velocity of the flow — since in a cylinder the volume, V, is equal to the product of the length into the cross-area. V = length X cross-area, or V length = fe cross-area Fig. 174. — Lud wig's stromuhr: a and 6, The glass bulbs; a is filled with oil to the mark (5 c.c.), while b and the neck are filled with salt solution or defibrinated blood; p, the movable plate by means of which the bulbs may be turned through 180 degrees, c, c, for the cannulas inserted into the artery ; *, the thumb screw for turning the bulbs; h, the holder. When in place the clamps on the arteries are removed, blood flows through c into a, driving out the oil and forcing the salt solution in b into the head end of the artery through c'. When the blood entering a reaches the mark, the bulbs are turned through 180 degrees so that 6 lies over c. The blood flows into b and drives the oil back into a. When it just fills this bulb, they are again rotated through 180 degrees, and so on. The oil is driven out of and into a a given number of times, each movement being equal to an outflow of 5 c.c. of blood. When the instrument has been turned, say, ten times, 50 c.c. of blood have flowed out. Knowing the time and the caliber of the artery, the calculation is made as described in the text. A modification of the form of this instrument has been de- vised by Tigerstedt.* We cannot, of course, make the experiment in this simple way upon a living animal; the loss of so much blood would at once change the physical and physiological conditions of the circula- tion, and would give us a set of conditions at the end of the experi- *"Skandinavisches Archiv f. Physiol.," 3, 152, 1891. VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 437 merit different from those at the beginning. By means of the stromuhr however, this experiment can be made, with this alteration, how- ever, that the blood that flows from the central end of the cut artery is returned to the peripheral end of the same artery, so that the circulation is not blocked nor deprived of its normal volume of liquid. The instrument, as is explained in the legend of Fig. 174, measures the volume of blood that flows out of the cut end of an artery in a definite time. The calculation for velocity is made as follows: Suppose that the capacity of the bulb is 5 c.c, and that in the experiment it has been filled 10 times in 50 seconds, — i. e., the bulbs have been reversed 10 times; then obviously 10 X 5 or 50 c.c. have flowed out of the artery in this time, or 1 c.c. in 1 second. The diameter of the vessel can be meas- ured, and if found equal, say, to 2 mms., then its cross-area is izr1 = 3.15 X 1 = 3.15. Since 1 c.c. equals 1000 c.mm., the length of our cyl- inder of blood would be given by 1000 the quotient of ^-^ = 317 mms. So that the blood in this case was moving with the velocity of 317 mms. per second. Another instru- ment that has been employed for the same purpose is the dromograph or hemodromograph of Chauveau. This instrument is represented in the accompanying figure (Fig. 175). A rigid tube (p-c) is placed in the course of the artery to be examined. This tube is provided with an offset (a) the opening of which is closed with rubber dam (m). The rubber dam is pierced by a needle the lower end of which terminates in a small plate lying in the tube (pi). When the instrument is in place and the blood is allowed to stream through the tube, it deflects the needle, which turns on its insertion through the rubber as a ful- crum. The angle of deflection of the free end of the needle may be measured directly upon a scale or it may be transmitted through tambours and recorded upon a kymographion. The in- strument must, of course, be graduated by passing through it cur- > Fig. 175. — Chauveau's hemodromo- graph (after Langendorff). The tube, p-c, is placed in the course of an ar- tery, the blood after removal of clamps flowing in the direction shown by the arrow. The current strikes the plate, a pi, and forces it to an angle varyin with the velocity. The movement pi is transmitted through the stem, n, which moves in a rubber membrane, m. The angular movement of the projecting end of n may be measured directly or may be made to act upon a tambour, as shown in the figure, and thus be transmitted to a recording drum. 438 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. rents of known velocity, so that the angle of deflection may be expressed in terms of absolute velocities. It possesses the great advantage over the stromuhr that it gives not simply the average velocity during a given time, but also the variations in velocity coincident with the heart beat or other changes that may occur during the period of observation. Efforts have been made to devise a method for the determination of the velocity of the blood-flow in the arteries of man. The method used, however, depends upon certain assumptions that are not entirely certain and the re- sults obtained, therefore, can not be used with confidence. The principle of the method consists * in determining the volume of the arm by placing it in a plethysmograph. Assuming that the outflow from the veins is constant in the part of the arm inclosed, then the variations in volume of the arm may be referred to the greater inflow of blood into this part through the arteries. The curve showing the variations in volume may, therefore, under proper conditions, be interpreted in terms of velocity changes. Mean Velocity of the Blood-flow in the Arteries, Veins, and Capillaries. — Actual determinations of the average velocity in the large arteries and veins give such results as the following: Carotid of horse (Volkmann), 300 mms. per second; (Chauveau) 297 mms. Carotid of the dog (Vierordt), 260 mms. The flow in the carotid, as in the other large arteries, is not, however, uniform; there is a marked acceleration or pulse at each systole of the heart during which the velocity is greatly augmented. Thus, in the carotid of the horse it has been shown by the hemo- dromograph that during the systole the velocity may reach 520 mms. and may fall to 150 mms. during the diastole. It is found, also, that this difference between the systolic velocity and the diastolic velocity tends to disappear as the arteries become smaller, and, as was said above, disappears altogether in the capillaries, in which the pulse caused by the heart beat is lacking. The smaller the artery, therefore, the more uniform is the movement of the blood. The flow in the large veins is approximately equal to that in arteries of the same size. In the jugular vein of the dog, for instance, Vierordt found a velocity of 225 mms., while in the carotid of the same animal the average velocity was 260 mms. In the capillaries, however, the velocity is relatively very small. From direct observations made by means of the microscope and from indirect observations in the case of man the capillary velocity is estimated as lying between 0.5 mm. and 0.9 mm. per sec. Vierordt reports some interesting calculations upon the velocity of the blood, in the capillaries of his own eye. Under suitable conditions,! the movements of the corpuscles in the retina may be perceived in consequence of the shadows that they throw upon the rods and cones. The visual images * Von Kries, "Archiv f. Physiologie," 1887, 279; also Abeles, ibid., 1892, 22. f "Archiv f. physiologische Heilkunde," 15, 255, 1856. VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 439 thus produced may be projected upon a surface at a known distance from the eye and the space traversed in a given time may be observed. The distance actually covered upon the retina may then be calculated by the following con- struction, in which A-B = the distance traveled by the projected image; A-n, the distance of the surface from the eye; and a-n, the distance of the retina from the nodal point of the eye. We have then the proportion ab : an :: AB:An, or ab = AB * an- An According to this method, Vierordt calculates that the velocity of the blood in the human capillaries is equal to about 0.6 to 0.9 mm. per second. In the arteries, more- over, it may be observed that the average velocity diminishes the farther one goes from the heart, — that is, the smaller the artery, — and reaches its minimum when the arteries pass into the capillaries. Thus, Volkmann reports for the horse the following figures: Ca- rotid, 300 mms.; maxillary, 232; metatarsal, 56 mms. In the veins also the same fact holds. The smaller the vein — that is, the nearer it is to the capillary region — the smaller is its velocity, the maxi- mum velocity being found in the vena cava. The general relations of the velocity of the blood in the arteries, capillaries, and veins igrs struction used to determine the size of the retinal image when the size of the external object is known : n, The nodal point of the eye. See text. Fig. 177. — Schematic representation of the relative velocities of the blood-current in different parts of the vascular system: a, The arterial side, indicating the changes with each heart beat and the fall of mean velocity as the arterial bed widens ; c, the capillary region — the great diminution in velocity corresponds with the great widening of the bed ; v, the venous side, showing the gradual increase toward the heart. may be expressed, therefore, by a curve such as is shown in Fig. 177. Explanation of the Variations in Velocity. — The general rela- tionship between the velocities in the different parts of the vascular system is explained by the difference in the width of the bed in which the blood flows. In the systemic circulation the main stem, the aorta, branches into arteries which, taken individually, are smaller 440 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. and smaller as we approach the capillaries. But each time that an artery branches the sum of the areas of the two branches is greater than that of the main stem. The arterial system may be compared, in fact, to a tree, the sum of the cross-areas of all the twigs is greater than that of the main trunk. It follows, there- fore, that the blood as it passes to the capillaries flows in a bed or is distributed in a bed which becomes wider and wider, and as it returns to the heart in the veins it is collected into a bed that be- comes smaller as we approach the heart. Vierordt estimates that the combined calibers of all the capillaries in the systemic circula- tion would make a tube with a cross-area about 800 times as large as the aorta. If the circulation is proceeding uniformly it follows that for any given unit of time the same volume of blood must pass through any given cross-section of the system, — that is, at a given point in the aorta or vena cava as much blood must flow by in a second as passes through the capillary region, and that consequently where the cross-section or bed is widest the velocity is correspondingly diminished. If the capillary bed is 800 times that of the aorta, then the velocity in the capillaries is -g-^-g- of that in the aorta, — say, -g-J-g- of 320 mms. or 0.4 mm. Just as a stream of water flowing under a constant head reaches its greatest velocity where its bed is narrowest and flows more slowly where the bed widens to the dimensions of a pool or lake. Variations in Velocity with Changes in the Heart-beat or the Size of the Vessels. — While the above statement holds true as an explanation of the general relationship between the velocities in the arteries, veins, and capillaries at any given moment, the absolute velocities in the different parts of the system will, of course, vary whenever any of the conditions acting upon the blood-flow vary. In the large arteries, as has been said, there are extreme fluctuations in velocity at each heart beat; but if we consider only the average velocities it may be said that these will vary throughout the system with the force and rate of the heart beat, or with the variations in size of the caliber of the small arteries and the resulting changes in blood-pressure in the arteries. Marey* gives the two following laws: (1) Whatever increases or diminishes the force with which the blood is driven from the heart toward the periphery will cause the velocity of the blood and the pressure in the arteries to vary in the same sense. (2) Whatever increases or diminishes the resis- tance offered to the blood in passing from the arteries (to the veins) will cause the velocity and the arterial pressure to vary in an inverse sense as regards each other. That is, an increased resistance diminishes the velocity in the arteries while increasing the pressure, and vice versa. * "La Circulation du Sang," Paris, 1881, p. 321. VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 441 The Time Necessary for a Complete Circulation of the Blood. — It is a matter of interest in connection with many physio- logical questions to have an approximate idea of the time necessary for the blood to make a complete circuit of the vascular system, — that is, starting from any one point to determine how long it will take for a particle of blood to arrive again at the same spot. In considering such a question it must be borne in mind that many different paths are open to the blood, and that the time for a complete circulation will vary somewhat with the circuit actually followed. For example, blood leaving the left ventricle may pass through the coronary system to the right heart and thence through the pulmonary system to the left heart again, or it may pass to the extremities of the toes before getting to the right heart, or it may pass through the intestines, in which case it will have to traverse three capillary areas before completing the circuit. It is obvious, there- fore, that any figures obtained can only be regarded as averages more or less exact. The experiments that have been made, however, are valuable in indicating how very rapidly any substance that enters the blood may be distributed over the body. The method first employed by Hering (1829) was to inject into the jugular vein of one side a solution of potassium ferrocyanid, and then from time to time specimens of blood were taken from the jugular vein of the opposite side. The first specimen in which the ferrocyanid could be detected by its reaction with iron salts gave the least time necessary for a complete circuit. The method was subsequently improved in its technical details by Vierordt, and such results as the following were obtained : Dog, 16.32 seconds; horse, 28.8 seconds; rabbit, 7.46 seconds ; man (calculated), 23 seconds. The time required is less in the small than in the large animals, and Hering and Vierordt con- cluded that in general it requires from 26 to 28 beats of the heart to effect a complete circulation. Stewart has devised a simpler and better method,* based upon the electrical conductivity of the blood. If a solution of a neutral salt, such as sodium chlorid, more concen- trated than the blood, is injected into it, the conductivity is in- creased. If the injection is made at a given moment and a portion of the vessel to be examined is properly connected with a galvanometer so as to measure the electrical conductivity through it, then the instant that the solution of salt reaches this latter vessel the fact will be indicated by a deflection of the galvanometer. Using this method, Stewart was able to show that in the lesser circulation (the pulmonary circuit) the velocity is very great compared with that of the systemic circulation — only about \ of the time required for a complete circuit is spent in the lesser circulation. Attention may also be called to the fact that the important part of the cir- * "Journal of Physiology," 15, 1, 1894. ■ 442 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. culation, as regards the nutritive activity of the blood, is the capil- lary path. It is while flowing through the capillaries that the chief exchange of gases and food material takes place. The average length of a capillary is estimated at 0.5 mm. ; so that with a velocity of 0.5 mm. per second the average duration of the flow of any par- ticle of blood through the capillary area is only about 1 sec. The Pressure Relations in the Vascular System. — That the blood is under different pressures in the several parts of the vascu- lar system has long been known and is easily demonstrated. When an artery is cut the blood flows out in a forcible stream and with spurts corresponding to the heart beats. When a large vein is wounded, on the contrary, although the blood flows out rapidly, the stream has little force. Exact measurements of the hydrostatic pressure under which the blood exists in the large arteries and veins were first published by Rev. Dr. Stephen Hales, an English clergy- man, in his famous book entitled " Statical Essays, containing Haemosta ticks," 1733.* This observer measured the static pressure of the blood in the arteries and veins by the simplest direct method possible. After tying the femoral artery in a horse he connected it to a glass tube 9 feet in length. On opening the vessel the blood mounted in the tube to a height of 8 feet 3 inches, showing that normally in the closed artery the blood is under a tension or pressure sufficient to support the weight of a column of blood of this height. A similar experiment made upon the vein showed a rise of only 12 inches. Methods of Recording Blood-pressure. — Since Hales's work the chief improvements in method which have marked and caused the development of this part of the subject have been the application of the mercury manometer by Poiseuille f (1828), the invention of the recording manometer and kymographion by LudwigJ (1847), and the later numerous improvements by many physiologists, and latterly the development of methods for measuring blood-pressures directly in man. The Hales method of measuring arterial pressure directly in terms of a column of blood is inconvenient on account of the great height, large fluctuations, and rapid clotting. The two former disadvantages are overcome by using a column of mer- cury. Since this metal is 13.5 times as heavy as blood, the column which will be supported by the blood will be correspondingly shorter and all the fluctuations will be similarly reduced. Poiseuille placed the mercury in a U tube of the general form shown in Fig. 178, M. One leg was connected with the interior of an artery by * For an account of the life and works of this physiologist see Dawson, "The Johns Hopkins Hospital Bulletin," vol. xv, Nos. 159 to 161, 1904. f Poiseuille, "Recherches sur la force du coeur aortique." Paris, 1828. j Ludwig, "Mi'iller's Archiv f. Anatomie, Physiologie, etc.," 1847, p. 242. VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 443 appropriate tubing filled with liquid and when the clamp was removed from the vessel its pressure displaced the mercury in the limbs by a certain amount. The difference in height between the levels of the mercury in the two limbs in each experiment gives the blood pressure, which is therefore usually expressed as being equal to so many millimeters of mercury. By this expression it is meant that the pressure within the artery is able to support a column Fig. 178. — A, Schema to show the recording mercury manometer and its connection with the artery: M, The manometer with the position of the mercury represented in black (the pressure is given by the distance in millimeters between the levels 1 and 2; one-half of this distance is recorded on the kymographion by the pen, P) ; F, the float resting upon the surface of the mercury ; G, the cap through which the stem carrying the pen moves ; E, offset for driving air out of the manometer and for filling or washing out the tube to the artery ; R, the receptacle containing the solution of sodium carbonate ; c. the cannula for insertion into the artery; w, the washout arrangement shown in detail in B. B, The washout cannula: c, the glass cannula inserted into the artery; r, the stem connected with the reservoir of carbonate solution ; o, the stem connected with the manom- eter. The arrows show the current of carbonate solution during the process of washing out, the artery at that time being closed by a clamp. of mercury that many millimeters in height, and by multiplying this value by 13.5 the pressure can be obtained, when desirable, in terms of a column of blood or water. For continuous obser- vations and permanent records the height of the column of mercury and its variations during an experiment are recorded by the device represented in Fig. 178. The distal limb of the U tube in which the mercury rises carries afloat of hard rubber, aluminum, or some other substance lighter than the mercury. 444 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. The float in turn bears an upright steel wire which at the end of the glass tube plays through a small opening in a metal or glass cap. At its free end it bears a pen to trace the record. If smoked paper is used the pen is simply a smooth- pointed glass or metal arm, while if white paper is employed the wire carries a small glass pen with a capillary tube, which writes the record in ink. The tube connecting the proximal end of the manometer to the artery of the ani- mal must be filled with a solution that retards the coagulation of blood. For this purpose one employs ordinarily a saturated solution of sodium carbonate and bicarbonate. This tube is connected also by a T piece to a reservoir con- taining the carbonate solution, and by varying the height of this latter the pressure in the tube and the manometer may be adjusted beforehand to the pressure that is supposed or known to exist in the artery under experiment. By this means the blood, when connections are made with the manometer, does not penetrate far into the tube, and clotting is thereby delayed. In long observations it is most convenient to use what is known as a washout cannula, — the structure of which is represented in Fig. 178, B. When this instrument is attached to the cannula inserted into the blood-vessel one can, after first clamping off the artery, wash out the connections between the artery and the manometer with fresh carbonate solution as often as desired. By such means continuous records of arterial pressure may be obtained during many hours. Determinations of the pressure in the veins may be made with similar apparatus, but owing to the low values that prevail on this side of the circulation it is more convenient to use some form of water manometer and thus record the venous pressures in terms of the height of the water column supported. It should be added also that when it is necessary to know the pressure in any special artery or vein the connections of the manometer are made usually to a side branch opening more or less at right angles into the vessel under investigation, or if this is not possible then a T tube is inserted and the manometer is connected with the side branch. The reason for this procedure is that if the artery itself is ligated and the manometer is con- nected with its central stump, the flow in it and its dependent system of capil- laries and veins is cut off; the stump of the artery constitutes simply a continuation of the tube from the manometer and serves as a side connection to the intact artery from which it arises. Thus, when a manometer is inserted into the carotid artery the pressure that is measured is that of the innominate or aorta from which it arises, while a cannula in the central stump of a femoral artery measures the pressure in the iliac. A specimen of what is known as- a blood-pressure record is shown in Fig. 179. The exact pressure at any instant, in millimeters of mercury, is obtained by measuring the distance between the base line and the record and multiplying by two. The base line represents the position of the recording pen when it is at its zero position for the conditions of the experiment. It is necessary to multiply the distance between the base line and the record by two because, as is seen in Fig. 178, the recording apparatus measures only the rise of the mercury in one limb of the manometer; there is, of course, an equal fall in the other limb. The blood-pressure record (Fig. 179) shows usually large rhyth- mical variations corresponding to the respiratory movements and in addition smaller waves caused by the heart beat. The causes of the respiratory waves in pressure are discussed in the section on respi- ration. Regarding the heart waves or pulse waves the usual record obtained by means of a mercury manometer gives an entirely false picture of the extent of the variations in pressure caused by the heart beat. The mass of mercury possesses considerable weight and iner- tia, which unfits it for following accurately very rapid changes in pressure. When the pressure changes are slow, as in the case of the long, respiratory waves seen in the record, the manometer un- VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 445 doubtedly indicates their extent with entire accuracy. But when these changes are very rapid, as in the beat of a dog's or rabbit's heart, the mercury does not register either extreme in the variation, but tends to record the mean or average pressure. The full extent of the variations in arterial pressure caused by the heart beat can be Fig. 179. — Typical blood pressure record with mercury manometer: Bp, The record showing the heart beats and the larger curves due to the respirations (respiratory waves of blood-pressure) and still longer waves due to vasomotor changes; T, the time line, giving the time in seconds. The actual arterial pressure at any moment is the distance from the base line — that is, the line of zero pressure — to the blood-pressure line, multiplied by two. These values are indicated in the vertical line drawn to the right, which shows that the average pressure at the time of the experiment was 100 mms. Hg. The small size of the variations in pressure due to each heart beat is altogether a false picture due to the inertia of the mercury, its inability to follow completely the quick change. Each heart beat, instead of being smaller, should be larger than the respiratory waves. determined by other means (see below), and, if the knowledge thus obtained is applied to the correction of the record of the mercury manometer, the tracing given in Fig. 179 should have, so far as the heart beats are concerned, somewhat the appearance shown in Fig. 180. This latter figure gives a more accurate mental picture of the actual conditions of pressure in the large arteries, as influenced by 446 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. the heart beat. These arteries are, in fact, subject to very rapid and very extensive changes in pressure at each beat of the heart, and these changes are naturally more pronounced when the force of the heart beat is increased, — for instance, by muscular exercise. Systolic, Diastolic, and Mean Arterial Pressure. — As stated in the last paragraph, the arterial pressure in the larger arteries undergoes extensive variations with each heart beat. The maxi- mum pressure caused by the systole of the heart, the apex of the pulse wave, is spoken of as systolic pressure; the minimum pressure in the artery — that is, the pressure at the end of the diastole of the heart, or the bottom of the pulse-wave, is known as the diastolic pressure. In a dog under ordinary conditions of experimentation the systolic (lateral) pressure in the aorta may be as much as 168- mms., while the diastolic pressure is only 100 mms. In man the $- / ».-,. *k 482 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. ventrifclfe. Owing to the whorl made by the superficial fibers at this rgoiiit as they turn to pass into the interior (see Fig. 202, I), the ^syatole causes a rotation of the apex, which is thereby forced /more firmly against the chest wall. This rotation and (Te&fiJn of the apex during systole may be seen upon the exposed {f neajyof the lower mammals and has been described also for man incases in which the heart is covered only by the skin, owing to formation in the chest wall (ectopia cordis) or to surgical operations. The Apex Beat. — The apex of the heart rests against the chest wall at the fourth or fifth intercostal space, and here the systole may be seen and felt in consequence of a slight protrusion of the wall. Much discussion has ensued as to why this protrusion occurs during systole, since the apex is drawn toward the base and the volume of the heart is diminished by the output of blood. The fact seems to be explained satisfactorily by two con- siderations : The heart during diastole rests against the chest wall at its apex and a portion of its anterior surface, but causes no pro- trusion of the wall because the tenseness of this latter is sufficient to flatten or deform the softer heart muscle. During systole the hardened heart muscle, on the contrary, overcomes the now rela- tively less resistant integument. The rotation of the apex tends also to maintain the contact; so that, although the heart is short- ened in its long diameter, the extent of the movement is not sufficient to draw it away from the chest wall. In the second place, the discharge of the heart contents into the curved aorta by tending to straighten this tube causes a movement of the whole heart downward which counteracts the effect of the shortening in the long diameter. The apex beat is proof that the apex remains against the chest wall during systole and in mammals corroborative experiments have been made by running needles through the chest wall into the base and the apex of the heart. Such needles act as levers with a fulcrum in the skin, and from the movement of the projecting portion it has been shown that, while the basal portion of the heart moves downward during systole, the apex remains more or less stationary except for the lateral movements due to the rotation. The Cardiogram. — The apex beat may be recorded easily by means of appropriate tambours. Several instruments have been especially devised for this purpose and are designated as cardio- graphs. The cardiograph described by Marey is shown in Fig. 203. It consists essentially of a tambour inclosed in a metal box. The rubber membrane of the tambour carries a button which can be brought to bear, under a suitable pressure, upon the apex of the heart. The movements of this button cause pressure changes in THE HEART BEAT. ft* i the air of the tambour which are transmitted through tubing t >'•* : recording tambour and recorded on a kymographion. A simpi and effective cardiograph may be made by pressing a fu: against the skin over the apex and connecting the stem of ifok* Fig. 203. — Marey's cardiograph. The button on the tambour is pressed upon the chest over the apex. The movements are transmitted through the tube to the right to a recording tambour. funnel by tubing to a suitable recording tambour. The car- diograms obtained by such methods have been the subject of much discussion. The form of the curve varies somewhat with the instrument used, the way in which it is applied, the position of the heart apex with reference to the chest wall, and with the con- Fig. 204. — Two cardiograms from the same individual to show characteristic records: a, Beginning of systole; b-c, systolic plateau. — (After Marey.) ditions of the circulation, and it is often difficult to give it a correct interpretation. An uncomplicated form of the cardiogram is represented in Fig. 204, 7, and a curve more difficult to interpret in Fig. 204, 8. It should be borne in mind that the cardiograph curve 484 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. is partly a pressure curve and partly a volume curve, — that is, the changes in volume as well as the changes in pressure of the heart during systole will affect the instrument. The Intraventricular Pressure During Systole. — The best analyses of the details of the systole of the ventricle have been made by a study of the changes in pressure within the ventricle. For this purpose a tube filled with liquid is introduced into the cavity of the ventricle. A tube used for such a purpose is designated as a heart sound. For the right ventricle it is introduced through an opening in the jugular vein and pushed down until it lies in the ventricle. For the left ventricle it is introduced by way of the carotid or subclavian artery and in this case is forced through the opening guarded by the semilunar valves. The sound is then connected to a suitable recording apparatus by rigid tubing filled with liquid. The changes in pressure in the ventricle are extensive Fig. 205. — Synchronous record of the intraventricular pressure (V), and the aortic pressure (A) : S, The time record, — each vibration = T&5 sec. ; 0-5, corresponding ordi- nates in the two curves ; 1 marks the opening of the semilunar valves; 3 marks the closure of these valves and the beginning of diastole. — (Hiirthle.) and very rapid. To register them accurately the recording instru- ment must respond with great promptness and at the same time must be free from inertia movements. A mercury manometer, for instance, would be entirely useless for such a purpose, since the heavy mass of mercury could not follow accurately the quick changes in pressure. The recording manometer devised by Hiirthle (p. 449) seems to have met the requirements more satisfactorily than any other of the numerous instruments described. A typical curve obtained by means of the Hiirthle manometer is given in Fig. 205, V. (Consult also the classical curve obtained b}^ Chauveau and Marey from the heart of the horse [Fig. 201].) It will be seen that the pressure in the heart rises suddenly with the beginning of the ven- tricular contraction and a certain time elapses before this pressure is strong enough to open the semilunar valves. The moment that this occurs (1, on the ventricular curve in Fig. 205) is determined THE HEART BEAT. 485 by simultaneous measurement of the pressure in the aorta, it being evident that the pressure will begin to rise in this latter vessel the moment that the valves open. It is interesting to find that the yielding of the valves to the rising pressure in the ventricle is not indicated on the curve itself by any variation, — a fact which indicates that the valves open smoothly, and are not thrown back with a sudden shock. A very characteristic feature of the ventric- ular curve is its flat top, or plateau as it is called. In some cases the plateau slopes more or less upward, in other cases downward, depending, doubtless, on" the respective values of the force of the heart contraction and the aortic tension, for during the whole time of the plateau the semilunar valves are open and the ven- tricle is discharging a column of blood into the aorta. The different features of the ventricular systole as gathered from these pressure curves are expressed by Hiirthle * as follows : I. Systole, phase of contraction of the muscle fibers (0 to 3 in Fig. 205, V). (a) Period of tension (0 to 1), during which the auriculo- ventricular and semilunar valves are both closed and the heart muscle is squeezing upon the contained blood. This period ends at the opening of the semilunar valves. (b) Period of emptying (1 to 3). During this time the heart is empty- ing itself into the aorta and the intraventricular pressure remains above aortic pressure. It ends with the cessation of the contrac- tion of the muscle and the beginning of the rapid relaxation. II. Diastole, phase of relaxation and rest of the muscle fibers. (a) Period of relaxation from 3 until the 'curve reaches a horizontal. At the beginning of the relaxation the semilunar valves are closed, and from comparison with the aortic curve the instant of the occur- rence of this closure is placed at 4. (b) Period of filling. This period begins as soon as the auriculo-ventric- ular valves open and the stream of blood, which had been flowing into the auricle throughout the ventricular systole, is permitted to enter the ventricle. During this period of filling the ventricular pressure rises slightly as the heart becomes turgid with blood. This increase of pressure is indicated in most cardiograms by a gradual rise of the curve during this period. It is shown in the curve of Chauveau and Marey, given in Fig. 201. The Heart Sounds. — An interesting and important feature of the heart beat is the occurrence of the heart sounds. Two sounds are heard, one at the beginning, the other at the end of the ventricu- lar systole. The first sound has a deeper pitch and is longer than the second, and their relative pitch and duration are represented frequently by the syllables lubb-dup. According to Haycraft,f both tones from a musical standpoint fall in the bass clef, and are separated by a musical interval of a minor third. The sounds are readily heard by applying the ear to the thorax over the heart, but for diagnostic purposes the stethoscope is usually employed, and * Hiirthle, "Archiv f. d. gesammte Physiologie," 49, 84, 1891. t Journal of Physiology/' 11, 486, 1890. 486 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. this method of investigation by hearing is designated as auscultation. The importance of these heart sounds in diagnosis was first em- phasized by Laennec (1819), and since his time a great number of theories have been proposed to explain their causation. Indeed, the subject is not yet closed, although certain general views regard- ing their cause and the time of their occurrence are generally accepted. The second sound is found to follow immediately upon the closure of the semilunar valves. The usual view, therefore, is that the sound is due ultimately to the vibrations set up in these valves by their sudden closure. These vibrations are transmitted to the column of blood in the aorta (or pulmonary artery) and then to the intervening tissue of the chest wall. This view is made probable by a number of experimental results, some of the most important of which were brought out by Williams in a report (1836) of a committee appointed by the British Association for the special purpose of investigating the subject. It has been shown: (1) That the second sound disappears before the first sound when the animal is bled to death, and indeed as soon as the heart ceases to throw out a supply of blood sufficient to maintain aortic tension. It disappears also when cuts are made in the ventricles so that the blood may escape otherwise than through the arteries. (2) When the valves of the pulmonary artery and aorta are hooked back in the living animal the second sound is replaced by a murmur due to the rushing back of the blood into the ventricle, and if the valves are dropped back into place the normal second sound is again heard. (3) Similar sounds may be produced if the root of the aorta with its valves in place is excised and attached to a glass tube carrying a column of water. With such an arrangement, if the valves are held open for a moment and then closed sharply by the pressure of the column of water a sound similar to that of the second heart sound is heard. The physician uses this view of the cause of the second sound in auscultation, and it is evident that the nature of the sound or its replacement by murmurs will give useful testimony regarding the condition of the semilunar valves. The first heart sound has of- fered more difficulty. It occurs at or shortly before the closure of the auriculo-ventricular valves, and it would seem natural, therefore, to attribute it to the vibration of these valves when suddenly put under tension by the ventricular systole. Most authors, indeed, believe that this factor is at least partially responsible for the sound, — that is, that the sound contains a valvular element. But that this is- not the sole cause is shown by the fact that the bloodless beating heart still gives a sound at the time of the ventricular systole. Indeed, if the apex of the rabbit's heart is cut off, it continues to beat for a few minutes and during this time gives a first heart THE HEART BEAT. 487 sound. It is usually said, therefore, that the first heart sound is caused by the combination of at least two factors, — a valvular element due to the vibration of the auriculo- ventricular valves, and a muscular element due to the vibration of the contracting muscular mass. Accepting this view, there is a further difficulty in explain- ing the origin of the muscular element. According to some, it is due to the fact that the contraction of the muscle fibers is not simultaneous throughout the ventricle and the friction of the inter- lacing fibers sets up vibration in the muscular mass; according to others, the so-called muscular element is mainly a resonance tone of the ear membrane of the auscultator, — the shock of the contracting heart sets the tympanic membrane to vibrating. It seems useless to attempt a detailed discussion of these conflicting views, since no convincing statements can be made. Practically, the time at which the heart sounds occur is of great importance. A number of observers have recorded the time upon a cardiographic tracing of Fig. 206. — To show the time relation of the heart sounds to the ventricular beat (Marey) : V.D., Tracing of the ventricular pressure in the right ventricle of the horse. Be- low the two marks show, respectively, the time of the first and second sounds. The first occurs immediately after the beginning of systole, the second immediately after the begin- ning of diastole. the heart beat with results such as are shown in Fig. 206. The figure shows clearly the general fact that the first sound is heard very shortly after the beginning of systole and the second one immediately after the end of systole. The first sound is therefore systolic, and the second sound diastolic. A more exact and de- tailed study of the time relations of the heart sounds has been made by Einthoven and Geluk.* These authors obtained graphic records of the heart sounds. The sounds received first by a microphone were transmitted to a capillary electrometer and the movements of the latter were photographed. As one result of their work they give the schema shown in Fig. 207. It will be seen from this figure that the first sound begins about 0.01 sec. before the cardiogram shows the commencement of systole, and that for the first 0.06 sec. the sound is heard only over the apex of the heart (a-b). Over the * Einthoven and Geluk, " Archiv f. d. gesammte Physiologie, " 57, 617, 1894. 488 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. base of the heart (second intercostal space) the first sound is heard (b to c-d) just at the time when the semilunar valves are opened (bf), — that is, at the beginning of the period of emptying according to the classification given on p. 485. The first sound ceases long before the ventricular contraction itself is over, — a fact which would seem to indicate that the muscular element in the first sound is not a muscular sound, such as is given out by a contracting skeletal muscle. The beginning of the second sound seems to mark exactly the time of closure of the semilunar valves. The character and the time relations of the murmurs that accompany or replace the heart sounds form the interesting practical continuation of this theme; but the subject is so large that the student must be referred for this information to the works upon clinical methods. The Events That Occur during a Single Cardiac Cycle. — By a complete cardiac cycle is meant the time from any given Fig. 207. — Schematic representation of the relation of the heart sounds to the ventric- ular beat: C, The cardiogram; 1, to show the duration of the first heart sound; 2, the duration of the second heart sound; S, the time record, each division corresponding to 0.02 sec. In 1, a-a' marks the instant that the first heart sound is heard over the apex, and b-b' the moment that it is heard at the second intercostal space. — (Einthoven and Geluk.) feature of the heart beat until that feature is again produced. It may be helpful to summarize the events in such a cycle both as regards the heart and as regards the blood contained in it. We may begin with the closure of the semilunar valves. At that moment the second heart sound is heard and at that moment the ventricle is quickly relaxing from its previous contraction. Since the auric ulo- ventricular valves are still closed, the ventricles for a brief interval are shut off on both sides. The blood is flowing steadily into the auricles and dilating them. As soon as the ventricular relaxation is complete the pressure of blood in the auricles opens the auriculo- ventricular valves, and from that moment until the beginning of the auricular systole the blood from the large veins is filling both ventricles and auricles. The ventricles become more tense and the auriculo-ventricular valves are floated into position ready for closure. The auricular systole sends a sudden wave of blood into the ventricles, dilating them still further and THE HEART BEAT. momentarily blocking or retarding the flow from the large veins, whence the normal venous pulse in the jugular veins. The ven- tricular systole follows at once upon the auricular systole, the exact relations in this case depending somewhat upon the pulse rate. As the ventricle enters into contraction the auriculo- ventric- ular valves are tightly closed, the first sound is heard, and for a short interval the ventricular cavity is again shut off on both sides. Soon the rising pressure in the interior forces open the semilunar valves, and then a column of blood is discharged into aorta and pulmonary artery as long as the contraction lasts. During this interval the flow at the venous end of the heart continues, the blood being received into the yielding auricles. Indeed, this capacity for receiving the venous inflow dur- ing the comparatively long-lasting ventricular systole may be considered as one valuable mechanical function fulfilled by the auricles. The venous flow is never completely blocked and at the most suffers only a slight retardation during the very brief auricular systole. At the end of the ventricular systole the excess of pressure in the aorta and the pulmonary artery closes the semilunar valves and completes the cycle. Time Relations of Systole and Diastole. — The duration of the separate phases of the heart beat depends naturally on the rate of beat. Assuming a low pulse rate of 70 per minute, the average duration of the different phases may be estimated as follows: Ventricular systole = 0.379 sec. Ventricular diastole and pause = 0.483 " Auricular systole =0.] to 0.17 " Auricular diastole and pause = 0.762 to 0.692 " Einthoven and Geluk, in the investigation referred to above, measured the time intervals of systole and diastole during fifteen heart periods of a healthy man, and found that the time for the systole varied between 0.312 and 0.346 sec, while that for the diastole varied from 0.385 to 0.518 sec. Experiments by a number of ob- servers indicate that in the great changes of rate which the heart may undergo under normal conditions the diastolic phase is affected relatively much more than the systolic, as we should expect. The Normal Capacity of the Ventricles and the Work Done by the Heart. — Various efforts have been made to measure the normal capacity of the ventricles, but the determination has encountered many difficulties. Experiments and observations made upon the excised heart are of little value, since the distensible rails of the ventricles yield readily to pressure, and it is difficult >r impossible to imitate exactly the conditions of pressure that >revail during life. Nor is it certain whether normally the ventricles impty themselves completely during systole. The older observers 490 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. (Volkmann, Vierordt) attempted to arrive at a determination of the normal output of the ventricles by calculations based upon the velocity of the blood in the carotid and the width of the stream bed. From observations on many animals they arrived at the generalization that at each systole the amount of blood ejected from the ventricles is equal to about 4^ of the body weight. For a man weighing, say, 72 kilograms (158 lbs.) this ratio would give an output for each systole of 180 gms. (6 ozs.). More recent observers, however, have found this estimate too high. Howell and Donaldson* measured the output directly for the heart of the dog, making use of a heart isolated from the body and kept beating by an artificial circulation. The ratio of the output varied with the rate of beat; for a rate of 180 beats per minute it was equal to 0.00117 (sis) of the body weight; for a rate of 120 beats per minute it was equal to 0.0014 (totf)- This ratio is therefore about one-half of that proposed by Volkmann. Tigerstedt, from observations upon rabbits, obtained a lower ratio still (0.00042); but from his own results and those obtained by other workers he concludes f that an average valuation for the volume of blood discharged by each ventricle of the human heart is from 50 to 100 c.c. On this basis one may make an approximate estimate of the work done at each beat. Using Tigerstedt's figures, such results as the follow- ing are obtained: On the left side the heart empties its 100 c.c. against a pressure of 150 mms. Hg. (0.150 meter) and on the right side against a pressure of, say, 60 mms. Hg. (0.06 meter). The work done is calculated from the formula w=pr, in which p repre- sents the weight of the mass thrown out and r the resistance or mean aortic pressure. This latter factor must be multiplied by 13.6, the density of mercury, to reduce to a column of blood. Left ventricle, 100 gms. X (0.150 X 13.6) = 204.0 grammeters. Right " 100 " X (0.06 X 13.6) = 81.6 285.6 grammeters. To this must be added the energy represented by the velocity of the mass ejected into the aorta. Placing this velocity at 500 mms. (0.5 meter) for both aorta and pulmonary artery, the energy represented in mechanical work is estimated from the formula ^ in which p represents the weight of the mass moved, v the velocity of its movement, and g the accelerating force of gravity. Applying this formula we have for each ventricle y9g5 = 1.28 grammeters, or for both ventricles 2.56 grammeters, making a total of over 288 * Howell and Donaldson, " Philosophical Transactions," Royal Soc, Lon- don, 1884. t Tigerstedt, "Lehrbuch der Physiologie des Kreislaufes," p. 152, 1893. THE HEART BEAT. 491 grammeters of work. That is, the mechanical work done at each contraction of the heart is equal to that necessary to raise 288 gms. a meter in height. The calculations made by different authors as to the amount of blood discharged from each ventricle during systole may be tabulated as follows: Thomas Young 45 gms. Volkmann 188 Vierordt 180 Fick 50-73 Howell and Donaldson 75-90 Hoorweg 47 Zuntz 60 Tigerstedt 50-100 Plumier 70 Loewy and v. Schrotter .... 55 for weight of 72 kgms. it a (i ti a it tt tt 65 It 60-65 kgms. The Coronary Circulation during the Heart Beat. — The condition of the blood-flow in the coronary vessels during the phases of the heart beat has been the subject of much speculation and experiment, since it has entered as a factor in the discussion of several mechanical and nutritive problems that are connected with the physiology of the heart. According to a view usually attributed to Thebesius (1708), the flaps of the semilunar valves are thrown back during systole and shut off the coronary circulation, and therefore the coronary vessels, unlike those of other organs, are filled during diastole. In modern times this view has been revived by Briicke, who made it a part of his theory of the "self-regulation" of the heart beat. According to this view, the coronaries are shut off from the aorta during systole by the flaps of the semilunar valves, so that the contraction of the ventricle is not opposed by the distended arteries, while, on the other hand, the reinjection of these vessels from the aorta during diastole aids in the dilatation of the ventricular cavities. Experimental work has shown decisively that the part of this theory relating to the closure of the coronary arteries by the semilunar valves is incorrect.* Records of pressure changes in the coronary arteries during the heart beat made by Martin and Sedgwick and by Porter show that they are substantially identical with those in the carotid or aorta, and records of the velocity of the blood-flow made by Rebatel show that at the beginning of systole the flow in the coronaries suffers a sudden systolic acceleration as in the case of other arteries. During systole, therefore, the mouths of the coronary arteries are in free communication with the aorta. But the coronary system — arteries, capillaries, and veins — is in part imbedded in the musculature of the ventricles, and we should suppose that the great pressure exerted by the contracting muscu- * See Porter, "American Journal of Physiology," 1, 145, 1898, for dis- cussion and literature. 492 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. lature would at the height of systole clamp off this system and stop the coronary circulation. That this result really happens is indi- dicated by Rebatel's curves of the velocity of the flow in the coro- nary arteries. As shown in Fig. 208, the great acceleration (a) in velocity at the beginning of systole is quickly followed by a drop to zero (6) or even a negative value, — that is, a flow in the other direc- tion, toward the aorta. At the end of the first (relaxation) phase of diastole there is again a sudden increase in velocity (c), corre- sponding with the injection of the arteries from the aorta, and this again by a decrease at the end of the diastole at the time when the ventricular cavity is filled with venous blood under some pressure. Porter, moreover, has shown in an interesting series of experiments that when a piece of the ventricle is kept beating, by supplying it with blood through its nutrient artery from a reservoir at con- stant pressure, each systole causes a jet of blood from the sev- Fig. 208. — Simultaneous record of the blood-pressure (A) and the blood-velocity (B) in the coronary arteries (Chauveau and Rebatel) : a, Marks the beginning of the systole (there is a rise in pressure and in velocity) ; 6, marks a second rise of pressure (A) due to the closure of the coronary capillaries by the contracting ventricle (at this moment in B the velocity falls off rapidly) ; c, curve (B) shows an increase in velocity due to the open- ing of the small coronary vessels at the beginning of diastole. ered vessels at the margin of the piece. In fact, the rhythmical squeeze of its own vessels during systole accelerates effectively the coronary circulation. The volume of blood flowing through the heart vessels increases with the frequency or the force of the beat, since each systole empties the coronary system more or less com- pletely toward the venous side and at each diastole the distended aorta quickly fills the empty vessel. The Suction-pump Action of the Heart. — So far in con- sidering the mechanics of the circulation attention has been directed only to the force-pump action of the heart. All of the energy of the circulation, the velocity of the flow and the internal pressure, has been referred to the force of contraction of the ventricles as the main cause, and to certain accessory factors, such as the respiratory movements and the contractions of the skeletal muscles, as subsid- iary causes. It is possible, however, that the heart may also act as THE HEART BEAT. 493 a suction-pump, sucking in blood from the venous side in conse- quence of an active dilatation. According to this view, the heart works after the manner of a syringe bulb, which when squeezed forces out liquid from one end and when relaxed sucks it in from the other in consequence of its elastic dilatation. While this view has long been entertained, modern interest in it was aroused chiefly perhaps by the experiments of Goltz and Gaule, which showed that at some point in the heart beat there is or may be a strong negative pressure in the interior of the ventricles.* Their method consisted in connecting a manometer with the interior of the ventricle and interposing between the two a valve that opened only toward the heart. The manometer was thus converted into a minimum manometer, which registered the lowest pressure reached during the period of observation. By this method they and others have shown that in an animal (dog) with an opened thorax the pressure in the interior of the ventricles may be negative to an extent equal to 20, 30, or even 50 mms. of mercury. Moreover, by the use of some form of elastic manometer, such as the Hurthle instrument (p. 449), it has been shown that this negative pressure occurs at the end of the period of relaxation, at the time, therefore, at which it might be supposed to exert a marked influence upon the inflow of venous blood. It should be added, however, that a negative pressure can not be shown for every heart beat. It may be absent altogether or slight in amount, varying, no doubt, with the force of contraction and the condition of the heart. Physiologists have attempted to determine the cause of this negative pressure and the extent of its influence on the blood-flow. With regard to the first question, so many answers have been proposed that it is difficult to arrive at a satisfactory opinion. According to some, the heart tends to dilate at the end of its systole by virtue of its own elasticity, — that is, the elasticity of its own musculature or of the connective tissue contained in its substance; for example, beneath the en- docardium, in the walls of the arteries, etc. This view, however, finds little or no support from direct experiments made upon the fresh, living heart. If such a heart in a bloodless condition is squeezed by hand there is no evidence of an elastic recoil as in the case of a syringe bulb. Others have explained the negative pressure as due not to a simple elastic expansion, but to what may be called a physiological expansion, — that is, an expansion due to physiological processes, such as anabolic changes. Such a view, however, is at present more or less speculative and can not be con- clusively demonstrated. Still others have traced the expansion of * For a complete discussion of this subject and the literature see the ar- ticle by Ebstein, " Die Diastole des Herzens," in the " Ergebnisse der Physi- ologie," vol. iii, part n, 1904. 494 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. the ventricle and the resulting negative pressure to the sudden in- jection of the coronary system from the aorta at the beginning of diastole. The heart in contracting exerts a force greater than that of the blood in the coronary vessels, and probably, therefore, these vessels are emptied and their cavities obliterated in part. At the beginning of diastole they are reinjected with blood under a pressure of perhaps 100 mms. of mercury, and this fact seems to offer a probable explanation for a partial dilatation of the ventricular cavity and a production of negative pressure in the brief interval before the opening of the auriculo- ventricular valves. No view, however, has met with general acceptance, and the cause or causes that produce the negative intraventricular pressure are still a subject for investiga- tion. Regarding the second question proposed above, — namely, the extent of the influence of this negative pressure on the flow of venous blood to the ventricles, — much diversity of opinion also exists. Direct experiments made by Martin and Donaldson* indicate that this factor has little or no actual influence upon the venous flow. These authors used an isolated dog's heart kept beating by an artificial supply of blood. At a given moment the stream of blood into the vena cava was shut off and the auricle of the heart was brought into communication with a U tube filled with blood. It was found that the auricle took blood from this tube only so long as the pressure in it was positive. Although the heart continued to beat vigorously, whatever negative pressure was present in the ventricle was unable to suck any blood into the auricle from the U tube. Porter f also has shown that at the time of a strong negative pressure in the ventricle the auricle may give little or no evidence of a similar fall in pressure. It would seem most probable, therefore, that the negative pressure observed under certain conditions in the ventricles is a fleeting phenomenon, and disappears with the entrance of the first portion of the blood from the auricles. While it may be of value in accelerating the opening of the auriculo- ventricular valves, its influence does not extend to an actual suction of the blood from the veins toward the heart. Other authors, however, on theoretical grounds attribute more actual im- portance to the negative pressure as a factor in moving the blood. Occlusion of the Coronary Vessels. — The coronary vessels supply the tissues of the heart with nutrition, including oxygen, so that if the circulation is interrupted the normal contractions soon cease. The branches of the large coronaries form what are known as terminal arteries, — that is, each supplies a separate region of the * Martin and Donaldson, " Studies from the Biological Laboratory, Johns Hopkins University," 4, 37, 1887; also Martin's " Physiological Papers," Baltimore, 1895. t "Journal of Physiology," 13, 513, 1892. THE HEART BEAT. 495 musculature, and although anastomoses may exist they appear to be too incomplete to allow a collateral circulation to be estab- lished when one of the main arteries is occluded. The portion of the heart supplied by it dies, or to use the pathological term, under- goes necrosis. On account of the pathological interests involved — the known serious results that may follow occlusion of any of the coro- nary vessels or even any interference with the normal structure of the vessels — a number of investigations have been made upon animals to determine the effect of occluding one or more of the coronary vessels.* It would seem from Porter's experiments that the results of such an operation vary according to the size of the area deprived of its blood. When the arteria septi alone was occluded the heart was not affected, when the arteria coronaria dextra was occluded the ventricular contractions were arrested in 18 per cent, of the cases observed. Occlusion of the ramus descendens of the left coronary artery caused arrest of the ventricles in 50 per cent, of the cases, while occlusion of the circumflex branch of the same artery caused arrest in 80 per cent, of the cases. Ligation of three of the arteries caused stoppage of the heart in all cases. Fibrillar Contractions. — The arrest of the ventricles in the experiments just described followed immediately or within a short period, and the ventricle went into fibrillar contractions. In this curious condition the various fibers of the ventricular muscle, in- stead of contracting together in a co-ordinated fashion, contract separately and irregularly; so that the surface of the ventricle has the appearance of a vibrating, twitching mass. Such a condition in the ventricle is usually fatal, — that is, the musculature is not able to recover its co-ordinated movement. This condition may come on with great suddenness as the result of occlusion of the arteries, of injury to certain parts of the heart, or from strong electrical stimulation. Fibrillation of the auricles also occurs frequently under experimental conditions, but the musculature in this part of the heart seems to be able to return to its normal co-ordinated contractions with much less difficulty. The cause of the sudden change from co-ordinated to fibrillar contractions has never been satisfactorily explained. In this connection it is interesting to recall also that when any injury is done to either ventricle suf- Ificient to stop the contractions or cause fibrillation both ventricles stop together. It is not possible to stop one alone. This result is doubtless due to the fact that their musculature is, after all, one set of fibers common to both chambers. * For a description of results and the literature see Porter, " Journal of Physiology," 15, 121, 1893; also " Journal of Experimental Medicine," 1, 1, 1896. CHAPTER XXIX. THE CAUSE AND THE SEQUENCE OF THE HEART BEAT— PROPERTIES OF THE HEART MUSCLE. General Statement. — The cause of the heart beat has naturally constituted one of the fundamental objects of physiological inquiry. The various views that have been proposed in different centuries reflect more or less accurately the advancement of the science. With each new discovery of general significance a new point of view is obtained and the theories of the heart beat, like those of the other great problems of physiology, shift their standpoint from generation to generation. The general modern conception of this problem is referred usually to Haller (1757), who first taught that the activity of the heart is not dependent on its connections with the central nervous system. As we shall see, the heart beat is controlled and influenced constantly by the central nervous system, but never- theless the important point has been established beyond question that the heart continues to beat when all these nervous connections are severed. The central nervous system regulates the activity of the heart, but has nothing to do with the cause of its rhythmical contractions. The heart, in other words, is an automatic organ. When in 1848 Remak discovered that nerve cells are contained in the frog's heart it was natural that the causation of the beat should be attributed to this tissue. Subsequent histological work has demonstrated the existence of numerous nerve cells in the substance of the heart tissue of all vertebrates, and the view that the au- tomaticity of the heart is due in reality to the properties of the contained nerve cells was the prevalent view throughout the middle and latter part of the nineteenth century. In the latter part of the century an opposite view arose, — namely, that the muscular tissue of the heart itself possesses the property of automatic rhythmical contractility. Both these points of view persist to day. The theory that refers the automaticity of the heart beat to the contained nerve cells is designated as the neurogenic theory of the heart beat; the one that refers this property to the muscle tissue itself is known as the myogenic theory. Beyond this question lies the still deeper problem of the explanation of the automaticity itself, the cause or causes of the rhythmical excitation, whether occurring primarily in the muscle cells or in the nerve cells. 496 PROPERTIES OF THE HEART MUSCLE. 497 The Neurogenic Theory of the Heart Beat. — The literature upon this topic is very large.* The neurogenic theory has suffered some changes in its details since first proposed by Volkmann, particularly in the specific functions assigned to the different ganglia that exist in the heart. In general, however, the theory assumes that the excitation to each beat arises within the nerve cells, and since the cardiac cycle begins with a contraction at what may be called the venous end of the heart, — that is, at the junction of the veins with the auricles, — it is assumed that the excitation or inner stimulus arises in the nerve cells situated in this region. These cells constitute, therefore, what may be called the automatic motor center of the heart. The stimuli generated within it are transmitted through its axons first to the musculature of the venous end of the heart. The subsequent orderly march of this contraction, to auricles and then to ventricles, is also upon this theory usually attributed to the intrinsic nerve cells and fibers. Through a definite mechanism the impulses generated in the motor center are trans- mitted to subordinate nerve centers through which the auricles are excited, and then to other nerve cells lying in or near the auriculo- ventricular groove through which the ventricles are excited. In this form the theory assumes for the heart an intrinsic central nervous system, as it were, with a principal motor center in which the property of automaticity is chiefly developed and subordinate centers whose activity usually depends upon the principal center, but which may show automatic properties of a lower order if the connections between them and the main center are interrupted. This intrinsic nervous system is responsible not only for the spon- taneous origination and normal sequence of the beat, but also for its co-ordination. The many muscular fibers of the ventricle contract normally in a definite manner and sequence; so that the effect is summated. Under abnormal conditions the fibers may contract irregularly, giving the so-called fibrillar contractions of the heart, which are inco-ordinated. It may be said that this con- ception of the connections of the intrinsic nervous system rests mainly upon deductions from physiological experiments. The histological details regarding the connections of the nerve cells in the heart are not yet sufficiently known, but it can not be said at present that they give any positive support to such a view. In regard to e neurogenic theory the following general statements may be made : 1. Most of the very numerous facts known regarding the heart * For recent general presentations from different standpoints see Gaskell, article on " The Contraction of Cardiac Muscle/' in Schafer's " Text-book of Physiology," vol. ii, 1900; Langendorff, "Herzmuskel und intrakardiale In- nervation" in "Ergebnisse der Physiologie," vol. i, part n, 1902; and Cyon, " L'innervation du cceur " Richet's " Dictionnaire du Physiologie," vol. iv, 1900. y 32 498 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. beat and its variations under experimental conditions may be explained in terms of the theory, or at least do not contradict it. The same statement, however, may be made regarding the myogenic theory. Both theories may be applied successfully from a logical standpoint to the explanation of known facts. 2. No single fact is known which can be cited as positive proof that the nerves participate in the production of the normal beat of the vertebrate heart. The experiment by Kronecker and Schmey is sometimes given this significance. These observers have shown that, when a needle is thrust into a certain spot in the dog's ventricle, the regularly contracting heart falls suddenly into fibrillar contractions so far as the ventricles are concerned. The ex- periment is certainly a striking and interesting one. The needle may be thrust many times into certain portions of the muscu- lar mass without affecting the powerful co-ordinated contractions, but in the region specified by Kronecker a single puncture, if it reaches the right spot, causes the ventricle to fall into ir- regular fibrillar twitches from which it does not recover. The spot as described by Kronecker is along the line of the septum at the lower border of its upper third. The experiment frequently fails; and it would seem that there must be a definite and quite circum- scribed structure whose lesion produces the effect described. We have no evidence as yet what this structure is, and are therefore in no condition to make positive inferences with regard to the bearing of the experiment upon the origin of the heart beat. Recently Carlson* has described experiments upon the heart of the horseshoe crab (Limulus) which seem to show conclusively that in this animal the rhythmical contractions are dependent upon the intrinsic nerve cells. These latter are placed superficially, forming a cord that runs the length of the tubular heart. When this cord is removed the heart ceases to beat. There are reasons, however, which at present make it impossible for us to apply the results of this ex- periment to the vertebrate heart. The crustacean heart differs from the vertebrate heart in its fundamental properties; unlike the latter, it has no refractory period (see p. 504), can be tetanized, and gives submaximal contractions.! It is a tissue, therefore, that resembles in its properties ordinary skeletal muscle in the verte- brate, and, like this muscle, it seems to be lacking in automaticity. Carlson's experiments give, however, another instance of automatic rhythmicity in nerve tissue, and to that extent supports the neurogenic theory. The Myogenic Theory of the Heart Beat. — The myogenic * Carlson, "American Journal of Physiology," 12, 67, and 471, 1905. t Hunt, Bookman, and Tierney, " Centralblatt f. Physiologie," 11, 275, 1897, PROPERTIES OF THE HEART MUSCLE. 499 theory has been developed chiefly by Gaskell and by Engelmann. It assumes that the heart muscle itself possesses the property of automatic rhythmicity and that this property is most highly de- veloped at the venous end. This portion of the heart, therefore, contracts first and the wave of contraction spreads directly to the musculature of the auricle and thence to that of the ventricle. The quickly beating venous end sets the pace, as it were, for the entire heart. The nerve cells and nerve fibers that are present in the heart are upon this theory supposed to be connected with the extrinsic nerves through which the rate and force of the heart beat are regu- lated, but they are not concerned in the production of the beat. Many experimental facts have been accumulated which give probability to this view, and it has been adopted by many, perhaps most, of the recent workers in this field. Some of the facts that favor this theory are as follows: 1. The anatomical arrangement of the musculature of the heart is not opposed to such a theory. It was formerly stated quite positively that there is no muscular connection between the auricles and ventricles in the mammalian heart, but a number of observers have now demonstrated the existence of a muscular bridge, the auriculo-ventricular bundle, between the two chambers (see p. 479). 2. The fact that a contraction started at one part of the heart may travel to other portions through the intervening musculature may be said to be demonstrated. Thus, Engelmann has shown that if the ventricle in the frog's heart is cut in a zigzag fashion, so that strips are obtained which are connected only by narrow bridges, a stimulation applied at one end starts a wave of con- traction which propagates itself over all of the pieces. This and similar experiments scarcely permit of explanation on the supposi- tion that conduction from piece to piece is effected by a definite nervous mechanism. So too it has been shown that under certain conditions the normal auriculo-ventricular rhythm can be changed at will to a ventriculo-auricular rhythm. If, for instance, a ligature be tied around the frog's heart between the sinus venosus and the auricle (first ligature of Stannius) the auricle and ventricle cease to beat. In this quiescent condition a slight mechanical stimulus to the ventricle causes it to beat and its contraction is immediately followed by that of the auricle. Such an experiment makes it most probable that the contraction is propagated directly through the muscular connections. It is not possible at present to conceive that a definite mechanism of neurons should work thus in either direction. 3. There is much probable proof that the heart muscle tissue possesses the property of automatic rhythmical contractions. Ex- periments, initiated by Gaskell and since extended by numerous 500 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. observers, show that in the cold-blooded animals strips of heart muscle taken from various parts of the heart will under proper conditions develop rhythmical contractions. It is very improbable that each of these strips, no matter how made, contains its own resident nerve cells to act as a motor center. Histology does not warrant such an assumption, and we must believe that these results demonstrate an inherent property of rhythmicity in cardiac muscle, whether or not this rhythmicity is directly responsible for the normal beat. 4. It has been shown that in the embryo chick the heart pul- sates normally before the nerve cells have grown into it, and it is stated that in the hearts of a number of invertebrates no nerve cells can be found. Much weight can not be given, however, to negative evidence of this kind, since, in the first place, better technical methods may demonstrate the existence of such cells, and even if absent from the heart itself it is conceivable that they may be present in the surrounding tissue and send their fibers to the heart. It is evident from this brief and imperfect presenta- tion that it is not possible to claim that either the neurogenic or the myogenic theory is demonstrated, but most physiologists per- haps at present believe that the latter view is more in accord with the facts. Automaticity of the Heart. — As was said above, the ques- tion of the cause or causes of the automatic rhythmical con- tractions must be sought for whether the phenomenon turns out to be a property of the muscular tissue or of the nervous tissue of the heart. When we say that a given tissue is automatic we mean that the stimuli which excite it to activity arise within the tissue itself, and are not brought to it through extrinsic nerves. In the heart, therefore, we assume that a stimulus is continually being produced, and we speak of it as the inner stimulus. Experiment and speculation have been directed toward unraveling the nature of this inner stimulus. Most of the physiologists who have expressed an opinion upon the subject have sought an explanation in the composition of the blood or lymph bathing the heart tissue, or in the products of metabolism of the tissue itself. According to this last view, each contraction results in the formation of certain products which stimulate the muscle to a new contraction (Langendorff). Regarding this view there is nothing of the nature of direct ex- perimental evidence in its favor. No product of the metabolism of the heart tissue capable of exerting this stimulating effect has been isolated. In regard to the former view, that the inner stimulus is connected with a definite composition of the blood or lymph, there has been considerable experimental work which is of funda- mental significance. While the older physiologists paid attention PROPERTIES OF THE HEART MUSCLE. 501 mainly to the organic substances in the blood, it has been shown in recent years that the inorganic salts are the elements whose influence upon the heart beat is most striking. These salts are in solution in the liquid of the tissue, and are therefore probably more or less completely ionized. Attention has been directed mainly to the influence of the metallic ions, the cations, of which three are es- pecially important, — namely, the sodium, the calcium, and the potassium. The Action of the Calcium, Potassium, and Sodium Ions in the Blood and Lymph. — It has long been known that the heart of a frog or terrapin may be kept beating normally for hours after removal from the body, provided it is supplied with an artificial circulation of blood or lymph, so arranged that this liquid enters the heart through the veins from a reservoir of some sort and is pumped out through the arteries leading from the ventricle. It was first shown by Merunowicz, working under Ludwig's direction, that an aqueous extract of the ash of the blood possesses a similar action. Ringer afterwards proved that the frog's heart can be kept beating for long periods upon a mixture of sodium chlorid, potassium chlorid, and calcium phosphate or chlorid, and he laid especial stress upon the importance of the calcium. This work was after- wards confirmed and extended by Howell, Loeb, and others, who attempted to analyze the part played by the several ions.* If a frog's or terrapin's heart is fed with a solution of physiological saline (NaCl, 0.7 per cent.) it beats well for a while, but the beats soon weaken and gradually fade out. If in this condition the heart is fed with a proper mixture of sodium, potassium, and calcium chlorids it beats vigorously and well for very many hours. A solution containing these three salts in proper propor- tions is known usually as Ringer's mixture. The exact com- position has been varied by different workers, but for the heart of the frog or terrapin the following composition is most effective: NaCl = 0.7 per cent. KC1 = 0.03 " " CaCl = 0.025 " " The addition of a trace of alkali, HNaC03, 0.003 per cent., often increases the effectiveness of the solution, but it can not be considered an essential constituent in the same sense as sodium, potassium, and calcium. It has been shown, moreover, that even the mammalian heart can be kept beating for long periods when fed with a Ringer solution if provision is made for a larger supply * For literature and discussion see Howell, *l>^enca\i^Y¥palf^r%^:i^*v iology," 2, 47, 1898, and 6, 181, 1901. jf W^'lJ „' U S 1 J \ 1 1 , {! ! J *- •? •* $ ■ * ■ - *< • j ; :e 8| ^ * 1 -i ft Ur Sfy t$ Ur (,£ It S£ K IP it i? sz (7 It 6/ It SI e/ // 60 19 SO £0 is If ss £S /S 1+ U StrljV I. §.5 <£ ■ i SSS illll "W,'H ai to §"■2 § si fljT3 03 £ °5 „ « C 3 to O . o ffcfl 2w~~g .S.SiS-S,© » > <-H *i |S -W The rate of the heart beat may be influenced also by many sub- stances added to the blood. The influence of atropin and muscarin have already been alluded to, but changes also in the normal con- stituents of the blood may have similar effects. Thus, an increase in the sodium carbonate of the blood affects the heart beat, particu- larly, however, in regard to the amplitude or force of the contraction. * Martin and Applegarth, Johns Hopkins University," 4, Papers/' p. 97, 1895. Studies from the Biological Laboratory, 275, 1890; also " Collected Physiological CHAPTER XXXII. THE VASOMOTOR NERVES AND THEIR PHYSIO- LOGICAL ACTIVITY. During the first half of the nineteenth century the physical or mechanical conditions of the circulation were carefully studied and great emphasis was laid upon such properties as the elasticity of the coats of the vessels. The physical adaptability thereby con- ferred upon the vascular tubes was thought to be sufficient for the purposes of the circulation. We now know that many of the blood- vessels are supplied with motor and inhibitory nerve fibers through whose activity the size of the vascular bed and the distribution of blood to the various organs are regulated. We know, also, that without this nervous control the vascular system fails entirely to meet what seems to be the most important condition of a normal circulation, — namely, the maintenance of a high arterial pressure. Although a number of physiologists had assumed the existence of nerve fibers capable of acting upon the muscular coats of the blood- vessels, the experimental proof of the existence of such nerves, and the beginning of the modern development of the theory of vasomotor regulation were a part of the brilliant contributions to physiology made by Claude Bernard.* In 1851 Bernard discovered that when the sympathetic nerve is cut in the neck of a rabbit the blood-vessels in the ear on the same side become very much dilated. He and other observers afterward showed that if the peripheral (head) end of the served nerve is stimulated electrically the ear becomes blanched, owing to a constriction of the blood-vessels. Thus the existence of vasoconstrictor nerve fibers to the blood-vessels was demonstrated. A vast amount of experimental work has been done since to ascertain the exact distribution of these fibers to the various organs and the reflex conditions under which they function normally. Few subjects in physiology are of more practical im- portance to the physician than that of vasomotor regulation; it plays such a large and constant part in the normal activity of the various organs. Bernard was doubly fortunate in being the first to demonstrate the existence of a second class of nerve fibers, which, when stimulated, cause a dilatation of the blood-vessels and which * See "Life of Claude Bernard," by Sir Michael Foster, 1899, in the series, "Masters of Medicine." 531 532 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. are therefore designated as vasodilator nerve fibers. This discovery- was made in connection with the chorda tympani nerve, a branch of the facial, which sends secretory fibers to the submaxillary gland. When this nerve is cut and the peripheral end is stimulated a secre- tion of saliva results and at the same time, as Bernard showed, the blood-vessels of the gland dilate; the flow of blood is greatly in- creased in the efferent vein and may even show a pulse. In the nervous regulation of the blood-vessels we have to con- sider, therefore, the existence and physiological activities of two antagonistic sets of nerve fibers: First, the vasoconstrictor fibers, whose action causes a contraction of the muscular coats of the ar- teries and therefore a diminution in the size of the vessels. Second, the vasodilator nerve fibers, whose action causes an increase in size of the blood-vessels, due probably to a relaxation (inhibition) of the muscular coats of the arteries. Before attempting to describe the present state of our knowledge upon these points it will be help- ful to refer to some of the methods by means of which the existence of vasomotor fibers has been demonstrated. Methods Used to Determine Vasomotor Action. — The simplest and most direct proof is obtained from mere inspection, when this is possible. If stimulation of the nerve to an organ causes it to blanch, the presence of vasoconstrictor fibers is dem- onstrated unless the organ is muscular and the blanching may be regarded as a mechanical result. On the other hand, if stimulation of the nerve to an organ causes it to become congested or flushed with blood the presence of vasodilator fibers may be accepted. It is obvious, however, that this method is applicable in only a few instances and that in no case does it lend itself to quantitative study. 2. Vasomotor effects may be determined by measur- ing the outflow of blood from the veins. If stimulation of the nerve to an organ causes a decrease in the flow of blood from the veins of that organ, this fact implies the existence of vasoconstrictor fibers, while an opposite result indicates vasodilator fibers. 3. By variations in arterial and venous pressures. When vaso- constrictor fibers are stimulated there is a rise of pressure in the artery supplying the organ and a fall of pressure in the veins emerging from the organ. This result is what we should expect if the constriction takes place in the region of the arterioles. The diminution in size of these vessels by increasing peripheral resistance augments the internal pressure on the arterial side of the resistance, and causes a fall of side pressure on the venous side (see p. 462). If the area involved is large enough the increased resistance will make a perceptible difference in pressure, not only in the organ supplied, but also in the aorta ; there will be a rise of general (dias- tolic) blood-pressure. On the other hand, a vasodilator action in THE VASOMOTOR NERVES. 533 any organ is accompanied by the reverse changes. Peripheral resistance being diminished there will be a fall of pressure on the arterial side and a rise of pressure on the venous side. When, therefore, the stimulation of any nerve brings about a rise of arterial pressure that can not be referred to a change in the heart beat the inference made is that the result is due to a vasocon- striction. When the method is applied to a definite organ — the brain, for instance — it becomes conclusive only when simultaneous observations are made upon the pressure in the artery and the vein of the organ, and proof is obtained that the pressures at these points vary in opposite directions. 4. By observations upon the volume of the organ. It is obvious that, other conditions remaining un- changed, a vasoconstriction in an organ will be accompanied by a diminution in volume, and a vasodilatation by an increase in volume. This method of studying the blood-supply of an organ is designated as plethysmography, and any instrument designed to record the changes in volume of an organ is a plethysmography Plethysmographs have been designed for special organs, and in such cases they have sometimes been given special names. Thus, the plethysmograph used upon the kidney and spleen has been desig- nated as an oncometer. The precise form and structure of a plethysmograph varies, of course, with the organ studied, but the principle used is the same in all cases. The organ is inclosed in a box with rigid walls that have an opening at some one point only, and this opening is placed in connection with a recorder of some kind by tubing with rigid walls. The connections between recorder and plethysmograph and the space in the interior of the latter not occupied by the organ may be filled with air, or as is more usually the case with water. The idea of a plethysmograph may be illustrated by the skull. This structure forms a natural plethysmograph for the brain. If a hole is bored through the skull at any point and a connection is then made with a recorder of some kind, such as a tambour, the volume changes of the brain may be recorded successfully. The plethysmograph generally employed in laboratories, particularly for in- vestigations on man, is some modification of the form devised by Mosso (see Fig. 220) . The hand and more or less of the arm is placed in a glass cylinder which is swung freely from a support. The opening around the arm is shut off by a cuff of rubber dam that must be chosen of such a size as to fit the arm snugly without compression of the superficial veins. The forward end of the plethysmograph is connected by tubing with a re- corder. Through appropriate openings the cylinder and connecting tubes are filled with warm water and then all openings are closed except the one leading to the recorder. Any increase in volume of the arm will drive water from the plethysmograph to the recorder, and any decrease, on * For a description of the development of this method, see Francois-Franck- Marey's "Travaux du Laboratoire, " 1876, p. 1. 534 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. the contrary, will suck water from the recorder into the plethysmograph. In the author's laboratory a modification that has been found most conve- nient is represented in Fig. 221. To avoid escape of water at the upper end of the tube and at the same time to prevent compression of the veins of the arm a very thin rubber glove with long gauntlet is used. The gauntlet is strengthened by cuffs of dam tubing, as shown in the illustration, and all are reflected over the end of the plethysmograph. The outer cuff (3) may be omitted. The hand is inserted into the cylinder and is held in place by flexing the fingers through the rings. The plethysmograph being suspended freely from the ceiling, any movement of the arm will move the instrument as a whole without disturbing the position of the arm in the instrument. By means of rings of hard rubber (D,E), one fitting around the rim of the plethys- mograph and the other adapted more or less closely to the size of the forearm, the reflected portion of the gauntlet and cuff is held in place and prevented from giving way readily to any rise of pressure in the plethysmograph. The Fig. 220. — A schematic diagram of Mosso's plethysmograph for the arms: a, the glass cylinder for the arm, with rubber sleeve and two tubulatures for filling with warm water; s, the spiral spring swinging the test tube, t. The spring is so calibrated that the level of the liquid in the test tube above the arm remains unchanged as the tube is filled and emptied. The movements of the tube are recorded on a drum by the writing point, p. interior of the latter is connected, as shown in Fig. 220, to a test tube swung by a spiral spring (Bowditch's recorder). The spring is so adjusted by trial that it sinks and rises exactly in proportion to the inflow or outflow of water. By this means the level of the water in the tube is kept constant, and since the posi- tion of this level determines the pressure upon the outside of the arm in the plethysmograph this pressure is also kept constant independently of the changes in volume of the arm. The level should be set in the beginning so as to make a slight positive pressure on the arm sufficient to flatten the thin glove to the skin and thus drive out the air between the two. When the apparatus is conveniently arranged, with slings to support the elbow, observations may be made upon the changes in volume of the arm during long periods. The results so obtained are referred to under several headings. With the form of recorder described the plethysmograph gives usually only the slow changes in volume of the arm, due to a greater or less amount of blood. By using a more sensitive recorder and making the con- THE VASOMOTOR NERVES. 535 nections entirely rigid the smaller, quicker changes in volume caused by the heart beat are also recorded. A volume pulse is obtained resembling in its general form the pressure pulse given by the sphygmograph. When used for this purpose the instrument is described as a hydrosphygmo graph. Records taken of the volume of the hand, foot, brain, or any other organ show that in addition to the changes caused by the heart beat and by the respiratory movements, there are other more irregular variations that are continually occurring the cause of which is to be found in the variations in the amount of blood in the organ. Day and night these changes in volume take place, and they are referable to the activity of the vasomotor system. Vasoconstriction or vasodilatation in the organ itself cause what may be called Fig. 221. — Detailed drawing of the glass plethysmograph with the arrangement of rub- ber glove to prevent leaking without compressing the veins. 2, The glove with its gauntlet reflected over the end of the glass cylinder ; 1 and 3, supporting pieces of stout rubber tub- ing ; D and E, sections of outer and inner rings of hard rubber to fasten the reflected rubber tubing and reduce the opening for the arm. an active change in volume. But vasoconstriction or vasodilata- tion in other organs may cause a perceptible change, of a passive kind, in the volume of the organ under observation. For, since the amount of blood remains the same, a change in any one organ must affect more or less the volume — that is, the blood contents — of all other organs. General Distribution and Course of the Vasoconstrictor Nerve Fibers. — These fibers belong to the autonomic system, and consist, therefore, of a preganglionic fiber arising in the central nervous system and a postganglionic fiber arising from the cell of some sympathetic ganglion. The general arrangement of the auto- nomic system (p. 231) should be reviewed in this connection. It has been shown by experiments of the kind described under the last heading that vasoconstrictor fibers are present in numerous nerve 536 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. trunks, but especially in those distributed to the skin and to the abdominal and pelvic organs. If, for instance, the sciatic or the splanchnic nerve be cut, to avoid reflex effects, and the peripheral end be stimulated, there will be a strong constriction of the vessels, which may be detected by ocular inspection, blanching; by the increase in arterial pressure; or by the diminution in volume of the organs. The vasoconstrictor fibers supplying these two great regions arise immediately (postganglionic fibers) from one or other of the ganglia constituting the sympathetic chain, or from the large prevertebral ganglia (celiac ganglion, for instance) directly con- nected with it. Ultimately, of course, they arise in the central nervous system (preganglionic fiber), and it has been shown that, for the regions under consideration, they all, with a few compara- tively unimportant exceptions, leave the spinal cord in the great Fig. 222. — Schema to show the path of the preganglionic and postganglionic portions of a vasoconstrictor nerve fiber: a, Anterior root, showing the course of the preganglionic fiber as a dotted line; d, v, dorsal and ventral branches of the spiral nerve; r, the ramus communicans; g, the sympathetic ganglion. The postganglionic fibers in each ramus come from the sympathetic ganglion with which it is connected. The preganglionic fibers enter- ing at any ganglion may pass up or down to end in the cells of some other ganglion. outflow that takes place in the thoracic region from the second thoracic to the second lumbar nerves (p. 233). In this outflow they are mixed with other autonomic fibers, such as the sweat fibers, pilomotor fibers, accelerator fibers to heart, pupilodilator fibers, visceromotor fibers, etc. Emerging in the anterior roots, they pass to the sympathetic chain by way of the corresponding ramus communicans. Having reached the chain, they end in one or other of the ganglia, not necessarily in the ganglion with which the ramus connects anatomically. The preganglionic fibers for the blood- vessels of the submaxillary gland, for instance, enter the first thoracic ganglion of the sympathetic chain, but do not actually terminate until they reach the superior cervical ganglion high in the neck. The postganglionic fibers arise in the ganglion in which the THE VASOMOTOR NERVES. 537 preganglionic fibers terminate. Those destined to supply the skin of the trunk and extremities pass from the ganglion to the cor- responding spinal nerve by way of the ramus communicans (gray ramus) and after reaching the spinal nerve they are distributed with it to its corresponding region (Fig. 222). In the general region Fig. 223. — Vasomotor effect of stimulation of the splanchnic nerve — peripheral end — in the dog (Dawson): 1. The line of zero pressure; 2, the line of the stimulating pen; on and off mark the beginning and end of the stimulation; 3, the time record in seconds; 4, the blood-pressure record (stimulation causes a marked rise of blood-pressure due to stimu- lation of vasoconstrictor fibers) ; 5, plethysmography tracing of the volume of the kidney (oncometer) j stimulation of the splanchnic causes a diminution in volume of the kidney owing to the constriction of its arterioles. Knder consideration (lower cervical to upper lumbar) each ramus ommunicans between a spinal nerve and a sympathetic ganglion onsists, therefore, of two parts, one (white ramus) of preganglionic ibers passing from the spinal nerve to the ganglion, the other (gray ramus) of postganglionic fibers coming from the ganglion to 538 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. the spinal nerve for distribution to the peripheral tissues. It should be borne in mind that the fibers in the white ramus do not return to the spinal nerve by the gray portion of the same ramus, but passing upward or downward in the sympathetic chain return to some other spinal nerve as postganglionic fibers. In this way, therefore, it happens that the various intercostal nerves and the nerves of the brachial and sciatic plexus contain vasoconstrictor fibers as postganglionic or sympathetic fibers. On the other hand, the vasoconstrictor fibers destined for the great vascular region of the intestines and other abdominal viscera, after reaching the sym- pathetic chain by way of the white rami as preganglionic fibers, do not return to the spinal nerves by the gray rami. They leave the sympathetic chain, still as preganglionic fibers, in the branches of the splanchnic nerves and through them pass to the celiac ganglion, where they mainly end, and their path is continued by the post- ganglionic or sympathetic fibers arising from this ganglion. More specific information concerning the origin of the vasomotor fibers to the different organs is given in condensed form farther on. It is quite important in the beginning, however, to obtain a clear general conception of the paths taken by the constrictor fibers from their origin in the spinal cord to their termination, on the one hand, in the vessels of the skin, or, on the other, in the vessels of the abdominal and pelvic viscera. The Tonic Activity of the Vasoconstrictor Fibers. — A very important fact regarding the vasoconstrictor nerve fibers is that they are constantly in action to a greater or less extent. This fact is demonstrated by the simple experiment of cutting them. If the sympathetic nerve in the neck is cut in the rabbit the blood- vessels of the ear become dilated. If the splanchnic nerves on the two sides are cut the intestinal region becomes congested, and the effect in this case is so great that the general arterial pressure falls to a very low point. From these and numerous similar ex- periments we may conclude that normally the arteries — that is, the arterioles — are kept in a condition of tone by impulses received through the vasoconstrictor fibers. Cut these nerves and the arte- ries lose their tone and dilate, with the result that, the peripheral resistance being diminished, the lateral pressure falls on the arterial side and rises on the venous side. The relatively enormous effect upon aortic pressure caused by paralysis of the tone of the arteries in the splanchnic area shows that under normal conditions the peripheral resistance in this great area plays a predominating part in the maintenance of normal arterial pressure, and by the same reasoning variations in tone in the arteries of this region must play a very large part in the regulation of arterial pressure. The Vasoconstrictor Center. — As stated in the last two para- THE VASOMOTOR NERVES. 539 graphs, the vasoconstrictor fibers emerge from the cord over a definite region, and they exhibit constant tonic activity. It has been shown, moreover, that if the cord be cut anywhere in the cervical region all of the constrictor fibers lose their tone; a great vascular dilatation results in both the splanchnic and skin areas. We may infer from this fact that the vasoconstrictor paths originate from nerve cells in the brain and that their tonic activity is to be traced to these cells. Such a group of cells exists in the medulla oblongata, and forms the vaso- constrictor center. The axons given off from these cells de- scend in the cervical cord and terminate at various levels in the anterior horn of gray mat- ter in the region from the upper thoracic to the upper lumbar spinal nerves. A spinal neuron continues the path as the pre- ganglionic vasoconstrictor fiber which terminates, as already described, in some sympathetic ganglion, whence the path is further continued by the post- ganglionic fiber. This arrange- ment of the constrictor paths is indicated schematically in Fig. 224. The exact location of the group of cells that plays the im- portant role of a vasoconstrictor eenter has not been determined histologically. Its region has, however, been delimited roughly by physiological experiments. If the brain is cut through at the level of the midbrain there is no marked loss of vascular tone in the body at large. If, however, similar sections are made farther and farther back a point is reached at which vascular paralysis begins to be apparent and a point farther down at which this paralysis is as complete as it would be if the cervical cord were cut. Between these two points the vasoconstrictor center must lie. The careful experiments of this kind made by Dittmar* are now somewhat old. According * " Berichte d. Sachs. Akademie, Math.-phys. Klasse, " 1873, p. 449. Fig. 224. — Schema to show the path of the vasoconstrictor fibers from the vaso- constrictor center to the blood-vessel and the mechanism for the reflex stimulation of these fibers: v. c, The vasoconstrictor center; 1, the medullary neuron on the vasoconstrictor path ; 2, the spinal neuron (preganglionic fiber) ; 3, the sympathetic neuron (postganglionic fiber) ; a, the arte- riole ; 4, the sensory fibers of the posterior root making connections by collaterals with the vasoconstrictor center; 5, an in- tercentral fiber (efferent) acting upon the vasoconstrictor center. 540 <\V £*BCULATTON OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. ^*—» — te-4*»*"Tfescription, the center is bilateral, — that is, .consists of a group of cells on each side, — and lies about the middle of the fourth ventricle in the tegmental region, in the neighborhood of the nucleus of the facial and of the superior olivary. In the rabbit it has a length of 3 mms., a breadth of 1 to 1.5 mms., and lies about 2 to 2.5 mms. lateral to the mid-line. Assuming the existence of this group of cells, we must attribute to them functions of the first importance. Like other motor cells, they are capable of being stimulated reflexly and by this means the regulation of the blood- flow is largely controlled. Moreover, they are in constant activity, — due doubtless also to a constant reflex stimulus from the inflow of sensory impulses. The complete loss of this tonic influence would result in a complete vascular paralysis, the small arteries would be dilated, peripheral resistance would be greatly diminished, and the arterial pressure in the aorta would fall from a level of 100-150 mms. Hg to about 20 or 30 mms. Hg, — a pressure insuffi- cient to maintain the life of the organism. There seems to be no question now that in those conditions known as surgical shock the loss of control by the vasomotor center, and the consequent vascular paralysis and fall of blood-pressure, are the chief conditions of a serious character. We must conceive, also, that in this vasocon- strictor center the different cells are connected by definite paths with the vasoconstrictor fibers to the different regions of the body ; that some of the cells, for instance, control the activity of the fibers distributed to the intestinal area, and others govern the vessels of the skin. Under physiological conditions the different parts of the center may, of course, be acted upon separately. Vasoconstrictor Reflexes — Pressor and Depressor Nerve Fibers. — It is obvious that such a mechanism as that described above is susceptible of reflex stimulation through sensory nerves, and according to our general knowledge we should suppose that, a tonic center of this kind may have its tonicity increased (excita- tion) or decreased (inhibition). Numerous experiments in phys- iology warrant the view that both kinds of effects take place normally. Those afferent nerve fibers which when stimulated cause reflexly an excitation of the vasoconstrictor center, and therefore a peripheral vasoconstriction and rise of arterial pressure, are frequently designated as pressor fibers, or their effect upon the circulation is designated as a pressor effect. Those afferent fibers, on the contrary, which when stimulated cause a diminution in the tone of the vasoconstrictor center and therefore a periph- eral vasodilatation and fall of arterial pressure, are designated as depressor nerve fibers, or their effect upon the circulation is a de- pressor effect. Pressor effects may be obtained by stimulation of almost any of the large nerves containing afferent fibers, but espe- \r THE VASOMOTOR NERVES. 5* cially perhaps of the cutaneous nerves. And there i of evidence to show that similar results can be obtained in man. The pressor effect manifests itself by a rise in general arterial pres- sure, if a sufficiently large region is involved, and by a diminution in size of the organ involved. On the other hand, depressor effects may also be obtained from stimulation of many of the large nerve trunks. If one stimulates the central end of the sciatic nerve, for example, one obtains a pressor effect on the circulation in most cases, but under certain conditions a marked depressor effect fol- lows the stimulation.* The simplest explanation of such a result is that the nerve trunks contain afferent fibers of both kinds. When we apply our electrodes to a nerve we stimulate every fiber in it and the actual result will depend upon which group of fibers exerts the stronger action, and this may vary with the condition artrtV «& ^^^MMt**** Fig. 225. — Plethysmography curve of forearm. The volume of the arm was recorded by means of a counter-weighted tambour and the record shows the pulse waves. A problem in mental arithmetic — the product of 24 by 43 — caused a marked constriction of the arm. of the nerve, the condition of the center, the anesthetic used, etc. Under normal conditions no such gross stimulation occurs. The pressor fibers are stimulated under some circumstances, the de- pressor fibers under others. For instance, when the skin is exposed to cold it is blanched not by a direct, but by a reflex, effect. The low temperature stimulates the sensory (cold) fibers in the skin, and the nerve impulses thus aroused reflexly stimulate the vaso- constrictor center, or a part of it, and cause blanching of the skin. Exposure to high temperatures, on the contrary, flushes the skin, and in this case we may suppose that the sensory impulses carried by the heat nerves inhibit the tone of the vasoconstrictor center and cause dilatation or flushing of the skin. So far as man is concerned, experiments made with the plethysmograph show very *See Hunt, " Journal of Physiology," 18,381, 1895. 542 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. clearly that the vasoconstrictor center is easily affected in a pressor or depressor manner by psychical states or activities. Mental work, especially mental interest, however aroused, is followed by a constriction of the blood-vessels of the skin, — a pressor effect (see Fig. 225) ; and we may find an explanation of the value of the reflex in the supposition that the rise of arterial pressure thus produced Fig. 226. — Effect of stimulating the central end of the depressor nerve of the heart. — ■ (Dawson.) The time record marks seconds. On and off mark the beginning and end of the stimulation. The blood-pressure rises slowly after the removal of the stimulus and eventually reaches the normal level. This complete recovery is not shown in the por- tion of the record reproduced. forces more blood through the brain (p. 560). On the other hand, feelings of embarrassment or shame may be associated with a de- pressor effect, a dilatation in the vessels of the skin manifested, for example, in the act of blushing. In both cases we must assume intracentral nerve paths between the cortex and the center in the medulla, the impulses along one path exciting the center, while those along the other inhibit its tone, or, as explained below, excite THE VASOMOTOR NERVES. 543 a vasodilator center. Among the many depressor effects that have been observed on stimulation of afferent nerve fibers one has aroused especial interest, — namely, that caused by certain afferent fibers from the heart. These fibers in some ani- mals— the dog, for instance — run in the vagus nerve, but in other animals, the rabbit, they form a separate nerve, the so-called depressor nerve of the heart, — discovered by Ludwig and Cyon (1866). In the rabbit this nerve forms a branch of the vagus, arising high in the neck by two roots, one from the trunk of the vagus and one from the superior laryngeal branch. It runs toward the heart in the sheath with the vagus and the cervical sympa- thetic. The nerve is entirely afferent. If it is cut and the peripheral end is stimulated no result follows. If, however, the central end is stimulated a fall of blood-pressure occurs and also perhaps a slowing of the heart beat (see Fig. 226). The latter effect is due to a reflex stimulation of the cardio-inhibitory center and may be eliminated by previous section of the vagus. The fall of blood-pressure is explained by supposing that the nerve, when stimulated, inhibits, to a greater or less extent, the tonic activ- ity of the vasoconstrictor center.* Anatomical studies show that in the heart the fibers arise in part at least in the walls of the ventricle, and physiological experiments indicate that the nerve plays an important regulatory role.f When, for in- stance, blood-pressure rises above normal limits it may be sup- posed that the endings of this nerve in the heart are stimu- lated by the mechanical effect, and the blood-pressure is thereby lowered by an inhibition of the tone of the constrictor center. It is possible, according to recent work, that the depressor fibers end in the walls of the aorta outside the heart. { In this position the effect of supranormal aortic pressures may more readily effect a stimulation of their endings and cause a fall of pressure. A similar nerve has been described anatomically in man, while in animals like the dog, in which it is not present as a separate anatomical structure, it probably exists within the trunk of the vagus. If this latter nerve is cut in the dog and the central end is stimulated a depressor effect is usually obtained. Vasoconstrictor Centers in the Spinal Cord. — From the description of the vasoconstrictor mechanism given above the probable inference may be made that throughout the thoracic region the cells of origin of the preganglionic fibers may, under special conditions, act as subordinate vasoconstrictor centers capable of giving reflexes and of exhibiting some tonic activity. Numerous experiments tend to support this inference When the spinal cord is cut in * See Porter and Beyer, " American Journal of Physiology, " 4, 283, 1900 ; also Bayliss, " Journal of Physiology," 14, 303, 1893. t Sewall and Steiner, "Journal of Physiology, " G, 162, 1885. t Koster and Tschermak, u Archiv f . die gesammte Physiologie, " 93, 24,. 1902. 544 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. the lower thoracic region there is a paralysis of vascular tone in the posterior extremities. If, however, the animal is kept alive the vessels gradually re- cover their tone, although not connected with the medullary center. The re- sumption of tone in this case may be attributed to the nerve cells in the lower thoracic and upper lumbar region, since vascular paralysis is again produced when this portion of the cord is destroyed. Finally, Goltz has shown that when the entire cord is destroyed, except the cervical region (p. 145), vascular tone may be restored finally in the blood-vessels affected. In this case the re- sumption of tonicity must be referred either to the properties of the muscular coats of the arteries themselves or to the activity of the sympathetic nerve cells that give rise to the postganglionic fibers. Under normal conditions it seems quite clear that the great vasoconstrictor center in the medulla is the important seat of tonic and of reflex activity. If the connections of this center with the blood-vessels are destroyed suddenly — for example, by cutting the cervical cord — blood-pressure falls at once to such a low level, 20 to 30 mms. Hg, that death usually results unless artificial means are employed to sustain the animal. Rhythmical Activity of the Vasoconstrictor Center. — Throughout life the vasoconstrictor center is in tone the intensity Fig. 227. — Rhythmical vasomotor waves of blood-pressure (Traube-Hering waves). The upper tracing (1) is the blood-pressure record as taken with the mercury manometer; the lower tracing (2) is taken with a Hiirthle manometer. Seven distinct respiratory waves of blood-pressure may be recognized on each large wave. — (Dawson.) of which varies with the intensity and character of the reflex im- pulses playing upon it. Under certain unusual conditions the center may exhibit rhythmical variations in tonicity which make themselves visible as rhythmical rises and falls in the general arterial pressure (Fig. 227), the waves being much longer than those due to the respiratory movements. These waves of blood- pressure are observed often in experiments upon animals, but their ultimate cause is not understood. They are usually desig- nated as Traube-Hering waves, although this term, strictly speak- ing, belongs to waves, synchronous with the respiratory move- ments, that were observed by Traube upon animals in which the diaphragm was paralyzed and the thorax was opened. THE VASOMOTOR NERVES. 545 These latter waves are also due to a rhythmical action of the vasomoter center. During sleep certain much longer, wave-like variations in the blood-pressure also occur that are again due doubtless to a rhythmical change of tone in the vasoconstrictor center. General Course and Distribution of the Vasodilator Fibers. — By definition a vasodilator fiber is an efferent fiber which when stimulated causes a dilatation of the arteries in the region supplied. In searching for the existence of such fibers in the various nerve trunks physiologists have used all the methods referred to above, — namely, the flushing of the organ as seen by the eye, the increased blood-flow, the increase in volume, or the fall in blood-pressure on the arterial side associated with a rise on the venous side. By these methods vasodilator fibers have been demonstrated in the following regions : 1. In the facial nerve. The dilator fibers are found in the chorda tym- pani branch and are distributed to the salivary glands (submaxil- lary and sublingual) and to the anterior two-thirds of the tongue. 2. In the glossopharyngeal nerve. Supplies dilator fibers to the posterior third of tongue, tonsils, pharynx, parotid gland (nerve of Jacobson). 3. In the sympathetic chain. In the cervical portion of the sympathetic dilator fibers are carried which are distributed to the mucous mem- brane of the mouth (lips, gums, and palate), nostrils, and the skin of the cheeks. These fibers pass up the neck to the superior cervi- cal ganglion and thence by communicating branches reach the Gas- serian ganglion and are distributed to the bucco-facial region in the branches of the fifth cranial nerve.* From the thoracic portion of the sympathetic vasodilator fibers pass to the abdominal viscera by way of the splanchnic nerves and to the limbs by way of the branches of the brachial and lumbar plexuses, but the data regarding the dilator fibers for these regions are not as yet entirely satisfactory. Goltz and others have shown that dilator fibers are found in the nerves of the limbs, but the origin of these fibers from the sympa- thetic chain has not been demonstrated. 4. In the nervi erigentes. Eckhard first gave conclusive proof that the erection of the penis is essentially a vasodilator phenomenon. The fibers arise from the first, second, and third sacral spinal nerves, pass to the hypogastric plexus as the nervi erigentes, and thence are dis- tributed to the erectile tissues of the penis. The General Properties of the Vasodilator Nerve Fibers. — Unlike the vasoconstrictors, the vasodilators are not in tonic activity; at least, no experimental proof has been given that they are. In the case of the erectile tissue of the penis and the dilators of the glands it would seem that the fibers are in activity only during the functional use of the organ, at which time they are excited reflexly. There has been much discussion in physiology as to the nature of the action of the dilator fibers. The muscular coat of the small arteries runs transversely to the length of the * See " Recherches experimentales sur le systeme nerveux vasomoteur, " Dastre and Morat, 1884. 35 546 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. vessel, and it is easy to see that when stimulated to greater con- traction through the constrictor fibers it must cause a narrowing of the artery. It is not so evident how the nerve impulses carried by the dilator fibers bring about a widening of the artery. At one time peripheral sympathetic ganglia in the neighborhood of the arteries were used to aid in the explanation, but, since histo- logical evidence of the existence of such ganglia is lacking, the view that seems to meet with most favor at present is as follows: The dilator fibers end presumably in the muscle of the walls of the arteries, and when stimulated their impulses inhibit the tonic contraction of this musculature and thus indirectly bring about a relaxation. Dilatation caused by a vasodilator nerve fiber always presupposes therefore a previous condition of tonic contraction in the walls of the artery, this tonic condition being produced either by the action of vasoconstrictor fibers or possibly by the intrinsic properties of the muscle itself. In the nerves of the limbs, as stated above, both vasoconstrictor and vasodilator, effects may be detected by stimulation. It has been shown that the separate fibers may be differentiated by certain differences in properties. Thus, if the peripheral end of the cut sciatic nerve is stimulated by rapidly repeated induction shocks a vasoconstrictor effect is obtained as shown plethysmographically by a diminution in volume of the limb. If, however, the same nerve is stimulated by slowly repeated induction shocks the dilator effect will predominate,* indicating a greater degree of irritability on the part of these latter fibers. After section of the sciatic nerve the vasodilators degen- erate more slowly than the vasoconstrictors, and they retain their irritability when heated or cooled for a longer time than the constrictors.f Vasodilator Center and Vasodilator Reflexes. — Since the vasodilator fibers form a system similar to that of the vasocon- strictors, it might be supposed that, like the latter, their activity is controlled from a general center, forming a vasodilator center in the brain similar to the vasoconstrictor center. What evidence we have, however, is against this view. In the dog with his spinal cord severed in the lower thoracic region the penis may show normal erection when the glans is stimulated, — a fact that indicates a reflex center for these dilator fibers in the lumbar cord. For the other clear cases of vasodilator fibers we have no reason at present to believe that they are all normally connected with a single group of nerve cells located in a definite part of the nervous system. The dilator fibers in the facial, glossopharyngeal, and cervical sympa- thetic (distributed through the trigeminal) all arise probably in the * Bowditch and Warren, ''Journal of Physiology," 7, 439, 1886. t Howell, Budgett, and Leonard, " Journal of Physiology, " 16, 298, 1894. THE VASOMOTOR NERVES. 547 medulla, but not, so far as is known, from a common nucleus. Intimately connected with the question of the existence of a general vasodilator center is the possibility of definite reflex stimulation of the vasodilator fibers. As stated above, reflex dilatation of the blood-vessels may be produced by stimulating various sensory nerve trunks. The depressor nerve fibers of the heart give only this effect, and the sensory fibers from certain other regions, notably the middle ear and the testis, cause mainly, if not exclusively, a fall of arterial pressure due presumably to vascular dilatation. The sensory nerves of the trunk and limbs, when stimulated by the gross methods of the laboratory, give either reflex vasoconstriction or reflex vasodilatation, and, as was stated above, there is reason to believe that these trunks contain two kinds of sensory fibers, — the pressor and the depressor. The action of the former predominates usually, but in deep anesthesia, and particularly in those conditions of exposure and exhaustion that precede the appearance of " shock," the depressor effect is most marked, or indeed may be the only one obtained. To explain such depressor effects we have two possible theories. They may be due to reflex excitation of the centers giving origin to the vasodilator fibers, or to reflex inhibition of the tonic activity of the vasoconstrictor centers. The latter explana- tion is the one usually given, especially for the typical and perhaps special effect of the depressor nerve of the heart. This explanation seems justified by the general consideration that in the two great vascular areas through whose variations in capacity the blood-flow is chiefly regulated, — namely, the abdominal viscera and the skin, — the vasoconstrictor fibers are chiefly in evidence, and are, moreover, in constant tonic activity. On the other hand, the fact that vaso- dilator fibers exist is presumptive evidence that they are stimulated reflexly, since it is by this means only that they can normally affect the blood-vessels. So that some of the many depressor effects occurring in the body must be due to reflex stimulation of the dila- tors and others to reflex inhibition of the constrictors. It would be convenient to retain the name depressor for the sensory fibers caus- ing the latter effect, and to designate those of the former class by a different name, such as reflex vasodilator fibers.* Only experi- mental work can determine positively to which effect any given reflex dilatation is due, but provisionally at least it would seem justifiable to assume that dilatation by reflex stimulation of the vasodilator fibers occurs in those parts of the body in which vaso- dilator fibers are known to exist. Thus, the erection of the penis from stimulation of the glans may be explained in this way, also the congestion of the salivary glands during activity, the blushing of the face from emotions, and possibly the dilatation in the skeletal muscles 548 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. during contraction. Gaskell and others have given reasons for believing that the vessels in the muscles are supplied with vaso- dilator nerve fibers, and Kleen* has shown that mechanical stimula- tion of the muscles — kneading, massage, etc. — causes a fall of arte- rial pressure. Vasodilatation Due to Antidromic Impulses. — The existence of definite effer- ent vasodilator fibers in the nerve trunks to the limbs has been made doubt- ful by the work of Bayliss. This author has discovered certain facts which at present tend to make the question of vasodilatation more obscure, but which, when fully understood, will doubtless give us a much deeper insight into the subject. Briefly stated, he has shownf that stimulation of the posterior roots of the nerves supplying the lumbo-sacral and the brachial plexus causes vas- cular dilatation in the corresponding limbs. He has shown that the fibers involved are sensory fibers from the limbs and that therefore when stimulated they must conduct the impulses in a direction opposite to the normal, — anti- dromic. It is most difficult to understand how such impulses, conveyed to the terminations of the sensory fibers, can affect the muscular tissue of the blood- vessels. It is most difficult to understand also how such anatomically afferent fibers can be stimulated reflexly in the central nervous system. Bayliss gives reasons for believing that the limbs receive no vasodilator fibers via the sym- pathetic system, and that either the blood-vessels in this region are lacking altogether in such fibers or else the sensory fibers function in the way described. General Schema. — The main facts regarding the vasomotor apparatus may be summarized briefly in tabular form as follows: I. Vasoconstrictor fibers — distributed mainly to the skin and the abdominal viscera (splanch- nic area) , all connected with a general center in the medulla oblongata, and in constant tonic activity, nerve libers. } n Vasodilator fibers— distributed especially to the erectile tissue, glands, bucco-facial region, and muscles; not connected with a general center and not in tonic activity. I. Pressor fibers. Cause vascular constriction and rise of arterial pressure from reflex stimula- tion of the vasoconstrictor center — e. g., sensory nerves of skin. II. Depressor fibers. Cause vascular dilatation and . ~, , ,, . • fall of arterial pressure from reflex inhibition ,fQr^^flflgo™g < of the tonic activity of the vasoconstrictor center, — e. g., depressor nerve of heart. III. Depressor (or reflex vasodilator) fibers. Cause vascular dilatation and fall of arterial pres- sure from stimulation of the vasodilator center, — e. g., erectile tissue, congestion of glands in functional activity. It may be supposed that under normal conditions the activity of this mechanism is adjusted so as to control the blood-flow through the different organs in proportion to their needs. When the blood- vessels of a given organ are constricted the flow through that organ * Kleen, " Skandinavisches Archiv f . Physiologie," 247, 1887. f Bayliss, " Journal of Physiology, " 26, 173, 1900, and 28, 276, 1902. Efferent vasomotor vasomotor reflexes. THE VASOMOTOR NERVES. 549 is diminished, while that through the rest of the body is increased to a greater or less extent corresponding to the size of the area in- volved in the constriction. When the blood-vessels of a given organ are dilated the blood-flow through that organ is increased and that through the rest of the body diminished more or less. The adaptability of the vascular system is wonderfully complete, and is worked out through the reflex activity of the nervous system exerted partly upon the vasomotor fibers and partly upon the regu- latory nerves of the heart. CHAPTER XXXIII. THE VASOMOTOR SUPPLY OF THE DIFFERENT ORGANS. There are three important organs of the body — namely, the heart, the lungs, and the brain — in which the existence of a vaso- motor supply is still a matter of uncertainty. A very great deal of investigation has been attempted with reference to these organs, but the technical difficulties in each case are so great that no entirely satisfactory conclusion has been reached. A brief review of some of the experimental work on record will suffice to make evident the present condition of our knowledge. Vasomotors of the Heart. — The coronary vessels lie in or on the musculature of the heart. Any variation in the force of con- traction or tonicity of the heart muscle itself will therefore affect possibly the caliber of the arterioles and the rate of blood-flow in the coronary system. At each contraction of the ventricles the coro- nary circulation is probably interrupted by a compression of the smaller arteries and veins, and the size of these vessels during diastole will naturally vary with the extent of relaxation of the cardiac muscle. Since stimulation of either of the nerves supplying the heart, vagus and sympathetic, affects the condition of the mus- culature, it is evident at once how difficult it is to distinguish a simultaneous effect upon the coronary arteries, if any such exists. Newell Martin* found that stimulation of the vagus causes dilata- tion of the small arteries on the surface of the heart as seen through a hand lens. Moreover, when the heart is exposed and artificial respiration is stopped the arteries may be seen to dilate before the asphyxia causes any general rise of arterial pressure. Martin interpreted these observations to mean that the coronary arteries receive vasodilator fibers through the vagus. Porterf measured the outflow through the coronary veins in an isolated cat's heart kept alive by feeding it with blood through the coronary arteries. He found that this outflow is diminished when the vagus nerve is stimulated, and hence concluded that the vagus carries vasocon- strictor fibers to the heart. MaasJ reports similar results also * Martin, " Transactions Medical and Chirurgical Faculty of Maryland," 1891. f Porter, " Boston Medical and Surgical Journal, " January 9, 1896. j Maas, "Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 74, 281, 1899. 550 VASOMOTOR SUPPLY OE THE ORGANS. 551 obtained from cats' hearts kept alive by an artificial circulation through the coronary arteries. Stimulation of the vagus slowed the stream, vasoconstrictor fibers, while stimulation of the sympa- thetic path quickened the flow, vasodilator fibers. Neither Maas nor Porter gives conclusive proof that the heart musculature was not affected by the stimulation. Schaefer,* on the con- trary, gets entirely opposite results. When an artificial circulation was maintained through the coronary system and the amount of outflow was determined he found that this quantity was not definitely influenced by stimulation of either the sympathetic or the vagus branches. Moreover, injection of adrenalin into the coronary circulation had no influence upon the outflow, and since this substance causes an extreme constriction in the vessels of organs provided with vasoconstrictor fibers the author concludes that the coronary arteries have no vasomotor nerve fibers. It is evident from a consideration of these investigations that the existence of vasomotor fibers to the heart vessels is still a matter open to investi- gation. Vasomotors of the Pulmonary Arteries. — The pulmonary circulation is complete in itself and, as was stated on p. 467, it differs from the systemic circulation chiefly in that the peripheral resistance in the capillary area is much smaller. Consequently the arterial pressure in the pulmonary artery is small, while the velocity of the blood-flow is greater than in the systemic circuit, — that is, a larger portion of the energy of the contraction of the right ventricle is used in moving the blood. From the mechanical conditions present it is obvious that the pressure in the pulmonary artery might be increased by a vasoconstriction of the smaller lung arteries, or, on the other hand, by an increase in the blood- flow to the right ventricle through the venae cava?, or, last, by back pressure from the left auricle when the left ventricle is not emptying itself as well as usual on account of high aortic pressure. While it is comparatively easy, therefore, to measure the pressure in the pulmonary artery, it is difficult, in the interpretation of the changes that occur, to exclude the possibility of the effects being due indirectly to the systemic circulation. Bradford and Dean,f by comparing carefully the simultaneous records of the pressures in the aorta and a branch of the pulmonary artery, came to the conclusion that the latter may be affected independently by stimu- lation of the third, fourth, and fifth thoracic spinal nerves, and hence conclude that these nerves contain vasoconstrictor fibers to the pulmonary vessels, the course of the fibers being, in general, that taken by the accelerator fibers to the heart, — namely, to the * "Archives des sciences biologiques," 11, suppl. volume, 251, 1905. f Bradford and Dean, " Journal of Physiology,' ' 16, 34, 1894. 552 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. first thoracic sympathetic ganglion by the rami communicantes and thence to the pulmonary plexus. They give evidence to show that these fibers are stimulated during asphyxia. The authors state, however, that the effects obtained upon the pressure in the pulmonary artery are relatively and absolutely small as compared with the vasomotor effects in the aortic system. Similar results have been obtained by other observers (Francois-Franck). Using another and more direct method, Brodie and Dixon* have come to an opposite conclusion. These authors maintained an artificial circulation through the lungs and measured the rate of outflow when the nerves supplying the lungs were stimulated. Under these conditions stimulation of the vagus or the sympathetic caused no definite change in the rate of flow, — a result which would indicate that neither nerve conveys vasomotor fibers to the lung vessels. This conclusion was strengthened by the fact that in similar per- fusions made upon other organs (intestines) vasomotor effects were easily demonstrated. Moreover, adrenalin, pilocarpin, and mus- carin cause marked vasoconstriction when irrigated through the intestine, but have no such effect upon the vessels in the lungs. These authors conclude that the lung vessels have no vasomotor nerves at all, and their experimental evidence might be accepted as satisfactory except for the fact that a similar method in the hands of another observer has given opposite results. Plunder f finds that the outflow through a perfused lung is diminished in some cases by stimulation of the sympathetic branches to the lungs, and also by the use of adrenalin. Under such conditions it is necessary to defer a decision until more experiments are reported. Regarding the vasomotors of the lungs, one can only say, as in the case of the heart, that their existence has not been demonstrated. The Circulation in the Brain and Its Regulation. — The question of the existence of vasomotor nerves to the brain brings up necessarily the larger question of the special characteristics of the cranial circulation. The brain is contained in a rigid box so that its free expansion or contraction with variations in the amount of blood can not take place as in other organs and we have to con- sider in how far this fact modifies its circulation. The Arterial Supply of the Brain. — The brain is supplied through the two internal carotids and the two vertebrals, which together form the circle of Willis. It will be remembered also that the vertebral arteries give off the posterior and the anterior spinal arteries, which supply the spinal cord, and that the last-named artery makes anastomoses along the cord with the intercostal arteries * Brodie and Dixon, "Journal of Physiology," 30, 476, 1904. t Plumier, "Journal de physiologie et de pathologie generate, " 6, 665, 1904; see also "Archives internationales de physiologie," 1, 189, 1904. VASOMOTOR SUPPLY OF THE ORGANS. 553 and other branches from the descending aorta. From the ana- tomical arrangement alone it is evident that the circulation in the brain is very well protected from the possibility of being inter- rupted by the accidental closure of one or more of its arteries. In some animals, the dog, one can ligate both internal carotids and both vertebrals without causing unconsciousness or the death of the animal. In an animal under these conditions a collateral circula- tion must be brought into play through the anastomoses of the spinal arteries. In man, on the contrary, it is stated that ligation of both carotids is dangerous or fatal. The Venous Supply.-— The venous system of the brain is peculiar, especially in the matter of the venous sinuses. These large spaces are contained between folds of the dura mater or, on the base of the skull, between the dura mater and the bone. The channel hollowed out in the bone is covered with a roof of tough, inex- tensible dura mater, and indeed in some animals the basal sinuses ^1 T)ura??2ater. ^J/racknoitl^ ->j (Jul 'arachnoids' Z^. Sftcvce. '~ Cerehrum. Fig. 228. — Diagram to represent the relations of the meningeal membranes of the cere- brum, the position of the subarachnoidal space and of the venous sinuses. may in part be entirely incased in bone. The larger cerebral veins open into these sinuses; the openings have no valves, but, on the contrary, are kept patent and protected from closure by the struc- ture of the dura mater around the orifice. The smaller veins are very thin walled and free from valves. The venous blood emerges from the skull in man mainly through the opening of the lateral sinuses into the internal jugular vein, although there is also a communication in the orbit between the cavernous sinus and the ophthalmic veins through which the cranial blood may pass into the system of facial veins, another communication with the venous plexuses of the cord, and a number of small emissary veins. In some of the lower animals — the dog, for instance — the main outflow is into the external jugular through what is known as the superior cerebral vein. A point of physiological interest is that the venous sinuses and their points of emergence from the skull are by their structure well protected from closure by compression. 554 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. The Meningeal Spaces. — The general arrangement of the menin- geal membranes, and particularly of the meningeal spaces, is im- portant in connection with the mechanics of the brain circulation. In the skull the dura mater adheres to the bone, the pia mater invests closely the surface of the brain, while between lies the arachnoid (Fig. 228). The capillary space between the arachnoid and the dura, the so-called subdural space, may be neglected. Between the arachnoid and the pia mater, however, lies the sub- arachnoidal space more or less intersected by septa of connective tissue, but in free communication throughout the brain and cord. This subarachnoidal space is rilled with a liquid, the cerebro- spinal liquid, which forms a pad inclosing the brain and cord on all sides. The liquid surrounding the cord is in free communication with that in the brain, as is indi- cated in the accompanying schematic figure (Fig. 229). Within the brain itself there are certain points at the an- gles and hollows of the differ- ent parts of the brain at which the subarachnoidal space is much enlarged, forming the so-called cisternse, which are in communication one with another by means of the less conspicuous canals (see Fig. 230) . The whole system is also in direct communica- tion with the ventricles of the brain on the one hand, through the foramen of Magendie, the foramina of Luschka, and perhaps at other places, and on the other hand, along the cranial and spinal nerves it is continued outward in the tissue spaces of the sheaths of these nerves. The Pacchionian bodies constitute also a peculiar feature of the sub- arachnoidal space. These bodies occur in numbers that vary with the individual and with age, and are found along the sinuses, especially the superior longitudinal sinus. Each body is a minute, jSHull. Brain-. Dura/ftater JLiouid- Column*. DuraJTlater. Cerebre Spinal lu/tud.. jjfrinal Cord. Fig. 229. — Diagram to show the connec- tion of the subarachnoidal space in the brain and the cord. VASOMOTOR SUPPLY OF THE ORGANS. 555 pear-shaped protrusion of the arachnoidal membrane into the inte- rior of a sinus, as represented schematically in Fig. 231. Through these bodies the cerebrospinal liquid is brought into close contact with the venous blood, the two being sepa- rated only by a thin layer of dura and the very thin arachnoid. The number of the Pacchionian bodies is hardly sufficient to lead us to suppose that they have a special physiological impor- tance. The cerebrospinal liq- uid found in the subarach- noidal space and the ventri- cles of the brain is a very thin, watery liquid having a specific gravity of only 1.007 to 1.008. It contains only traces of proteids and other organic substances, which may vary under pathological conditions. It is much thinner and more watery than the lymph, resembling rather the aqueous humor of the eye. The amount of this fluid Fig. 230. — Diagram to show the location of the cistemae and canals of the subarach- noidal space. — (Poirier and Charpy.) Fig. 231. — Schema to show the relations of the Pacchionian bodies to the sinuses: a, a, Folds of the dura mater, inclosing a sinus between them; v.b., the blood in the sinus; a, the arachnoidal membrane; p, the pia mater; Pa., the Pacchionian body as a projection of the arachnoid into the blood sinus. present normally is difficult to determine. Various figures have been given, but it is usually stated to amount to 60 to 80 c.c. If 556 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. these figures are correct it evidently does not form a thick envelope to the nervous system. Under abnormal conditions (hydroceph- alus, etc.) the quantity may be greatly increased. It is physio- logically interesting to find that this liquid may be formed very promptly from the blood and, when in excess, be absorbed quickly by the blood. In fractures of the base of the skull, for instance, the liquid has been observed to drain off steadily at the rate of 200 c.c. or more per day. On the other hand, when one injects physio- logical saline into the subarachnoidal space under some pressure it is absorbed with surprising rapidity. After death, also, the liquid present in the subarachnoidal space is soon absorbed. Intracranial Pressure. — By intracranial pressure is meant the pressure in the space between the skull and the brain, — therefore the pressure in the subarachnoidal liquid and presumably also the pressure in the ventricles of the brain, since the two spaces are in communication. This pressure may be measured by boring a hole through the skull, dividing the dura, and connecting the under- lying space with a manometer. Observers who have measured this pressure state that it is always the same as the venous pressure within the sinuses. This we can understand when we remember the close relations between the subarachnoidal liquid and the large veins and sinuses. We may consider that the large veins are sur- rounded by the cerebrospinal liquid, and consequently an equilib- rium of pressure must be established between them ; any rise in the intracranial pressure raises venous pressure by compression of the veins. This statement holds true at least so far as the intracranial pressure is due to the circulation. Variations of pressure from pathological causes — tumors, clots, abscesses, etc. — may exercise apparently a local effect. The intracranial pressure is caused and controlled normally by the pressure within the arteries and capillaries. This pressure, by enlarging these vessels, tends to expand the brain against the skull, and exercises a pressure, there- fore, upon the intervening cerebrospinal liquid. This pressure, however, can not exceed that in the veins, since, as said, an ex- cess will be equalized by a corresponding compression of the veins. The venous pressure in the end determines, therefore, the actual amount of intracranial pressure. Conditions which alter the pressure in the cerebral veins affect the intracranial pressure correspondingly. Thus, compression of the veins of the neck raises the pressure in the cerebral veins and also intracranial pressure, and a higher general arterial pressure also results finally in a higher pressure in the cerebral veins and therefore in the subarachnoidal space. Reduced to its simplest form, the conditions may be represented by a schema such as is given in Fig. 232. A system with an artery, capillary area, VASOMOTOR SUPPLY OF THE ORGANS. 557 and a vein is represented as inclosed in a rigid box and surrounded by an incompressible liquid. According to the conditions prevailing in the body, the pressure in the interior of A and its branches is much higher than in V. If, now, the pressure in A is increased the greater pressure brought to bear on the walls will tend to expand them; a greater pressure will thereby be communicated to the outside liquid, which, in turn, will compress the veins correspondingly. The expansion on the arterial side is made possible by a corresponding diminution on the venous side where pressure is least. the internal ^ ^\ % f* \ % f i/ -4 1 The recorded measure- ments of the intracranial pressure show that it may vary from 50 to 60 mms. of mercury, obtained during the great rise of pressure following strychnin poisoning, to zero or less, as obtained by Hill * from a man while in the erect pos- ture. In this position the negative influence of gravity is at its maximum. The Effect of Variations in Arterial Pressure upon the Blood-flow through the Brain. — Quite a number of observers f have proved ex- perimentally that a rise of general arterial pressure is fol- lowed, not only by an increase in the intracranial tension, but also by an increased blood-flow through the brain. There has been much discussion as to whether a rise of arterial pressure in the basilar arteries can cause any actual increase in the amount of blood in the brain or whether it expresses itself simply or mainly as an increased amount of flow. In the other organs of the body, except perhaps the bones, a general rise of pressure, not accompanied by a constriction of the organ's own arteries, causes a dilatation or congestion of the organ together with an increased blood-flow. Physiologically the congestion — that is, the increased capacity of the vessels — is of no value ; the important thing is the increase in the quantity * Bayliss and Hill, " Journal of Physiology," 18, 356, 1895. t See Gartner and Wagner, " Wiener med. Wochenschrift," 1887; de Boeck and Verhorgen, "Journal de Medecine, etc.," Brussels; Roy and Sher- rington, "Journal of Physiology," 11, 85, 1890; Reiner and Schnitzler, "Archiv f. exp. Pathol, u. Pharmakol. , " 38, 249, 1897. Fig. 232. — Schema to represent the transmission of arterial pressure through the brain substance to the veins: A, The artery, V, the vein, represented as entering into and emerging from a box with rigid walls and fijled with incompressible liquid; c, c, the intervening area of small arte- ries, etc. An expansion of the walls of the arterial system by the pulse wave or by a rise of arterial pressure increases the pres- sure on the surrounding liquid and this is transmitted through the liquid to the walls of the veins and compresses them, since at this point of the circuit the intravascular pressure is low. 558 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. of blood flowing through. In the brain, owing to the peculiarities of its position, it has been suggested that perhaps no actual increase in size is possible. It is evident, however, that the existence of the liquid in the subarachnoidal space makes possible some actual expan- sion of the organ. For as the pressure upon this liquid increases it may be driven into the dural sac of the cord (Fig. 229) and along the sheaths of the cranial and spinal nerves. To what extent this is actually possible in man we do not know, nor do we know how much cerebrospinal liquid is contained in the skull and brain of man. In the dog Hill * finds experimentally that the brain can expand only by an amount equal to 2 or 3 c.c. without causing a rise of intra- cranial tension ; so that probably these figures represent the amount of expansion possible in this animal by simple squeezing out of the cerebrospinal liquid. If the rise of arterial pressure is such as to expand the brain beyond this point, then it may not only force out cerebrospinal liquid, if any remains, but, as explained in the last paragraph, it will compress the veins and raise intracranial Fig. 233. — Simultaneous record of pulse in the circle of Willis (c) and in the torcu- lar Herophili (t). The tracing from the circle of Willis was obtained by means of a Hurthle manometer connected with the head end of the internal carotid. It will be noted that the pulses are simultaneous, indicating that the venous pulse is due to the transmis- sion of the arterial pulse through the brain substance. pressure. To the extent that the veins are compressed as the ar- teries expand no actual increase in the size or blood-capacity of the brain takes place. That an expansion of the brain arteries com- presses the veins is indicated very clearly by the normal occurrence of a venous pulse in this organ. The blood flows out of the veins of the brain in pulses synchronous with the arterial pulses, and this venous pulse may be recorded easily as shown in Fig. 233. In this case the sudden expansion of the arteries compresses the cerebral veins, giving a synchronous rise of pressure in the interior of the sinuses. Some authors (Geigel, Grashey), on purely theoretical grounds, have held that this compression of the veins may result in a diminished blood-flow through the organ, — a, sort of self-strangu- lation of its own circulation. Actual experiment shows that this is not the case. Any ordinary rise of general arterial pressure is accompanied by a greater blood-flow through the brain, and * Hill, "The Physiology and Pathology of the Cerebral Circulation. " London, 1896. VASOMOTOR SUPPLY OF THE ORGANS. 559 the author* has shown that sudden variations of arterial pressure far beyond possible normal limits cause no blocking of the venous outflow. Whether the brain increases in volume as a result of a rise of arterial pressure is, on the physiological side, unimportant; the main point is that the amount of blood flowing through it is increased under such circumstances as would cause a like result in other organs. That the compression of the veins does not produce any sensible obstruction to the blood-flow may be under- stood easily. In the first place, this compression does not take place at the narrow exit from the skull, — since at that point the sinuses are protected from the action of intracranial pressure. The compression takes place doubtless upon the cerebral veins emptying into the sinuses, and at this point the venous bed, taken as a whole, is so large that the expansion due to an ordinary rise of arterial pressure is distributed and has but little effect on the volume of the flow. Secondly, very great increases in arterial pressure, up to the point of rupture of the walls, have less and less effect in actually expanding the arteries ; a point is reached eventually at which these tubes become practically rigid, so that farther expansion is impossible. This, of course, is true for every organ. The Regulation of the Brain Circulation. — It is still a matter of uncertainty whether the arteries of the brain possess vasomotor nerves. Most of the authors who have studied the matter experi- mentally have concluded that there are none.f These authors were unable to show that stimulation of any of the nerve paths that might innervate the brain vessels causes local effects upon the brain circulation. Whenever such stimulations caused a change in pres- sure or amount of flow in the brain the result was referable to an alter- ation of general arterial pressure produced by a vasomotor change elsewhere in the body. When as a result of such stimulation the pressure rises in the circle of Willis, one may infer that if this is due to a local constriction in the cerebral arterioles there should be a fall of pressure in the venous sinuses and a diminished flow of blood ; if, on the contrary, it is due to a constriction elsewhere in the body that has increased general arterial pressure, but has not constricted the brain circuit, then there should be a rise in venous pressure and intracranial pressure, together with a greater flow of blood through the brain. Most observers obtain this latter result. Some inves- tigators,— Hiirthle, Francois-Franck, Cavazanni,J on the other * Howell, " American Journal of Physiology," 1, 57, 1898. t See Roy and Sherrington, Bayliss and Hill, Hill, Gaertner and Wagner, loc. cit., and Hill and MacLeod, ''Journal of Physiology," 26, 394, 1901. X Hiirthle, " Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologic/ ' 44, 574, 1889 ; Francois- Franck, "Archives de physiol. normale et pathologique," 1890; Cava- zanni, "Archives italiennes de biologie," 19, 214, 1893. 560 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. hand, have obtained results, especially from stimulation of the cervical sympathetic, which indicated local vasoconstriction or vaso- dilatation in the brain. It would seem, however, that these latter observers have not excluded the possibility that the variations in pressure obtained by them were due to reflex effects upon the blood-vessels of the body, especially as Francois-Franck has shown that the sympathetic in the neck contains afferent fibers which give such reflexes * As an argument in favor of the presence of vasomotor fibers it may also be mentioned that a number of observers — Gulland, Huber, Hunter f have demonstrated that the vessels of the brain are provided with perivascular nerve plexuses. It must be admitted, however, that this histological fact is not con- clusive unless it is supplemented by experimental evidence. Judged from this latter standpoint, we have no satisfactory proof at present of the existence of cerebral vasomotors, and, accepting this negative evidence, we may ask by what means is the circulation in the brain regulated? The simplest view is that proposed by Roy and Sherrington. According to these authors, the blood-flow through the brain is controlled indirectly by vasomotor effects upon the rest of the body. When, for example, a vasoconstriction occurs in the skin or the splanchnic area the result is a rise of pressure in the aorta and therefore a rise of pressure in the circle of Willis, which then forces more blood through the brain. Adopting this view, we can understand the teleology of certain well-known vaso- motor reflexes. Stimulation of the skin generally causes a reflex constriction and rise of pressure, and one can well understand that this result is valuable if it means a greater flow of blood through the brain, since under the conditions of nature such stimulation, especially when painful, demands alertness and increased activity on the part of the animal. Attention has also been called to the fact that in plethysmographic observations on man the most certain and extensive constrictions of the skin vessels are those caused by increased mental activity. Mosso has shown by observa- tions upon men with trephine holes in the skull that the constriction of the limbs is always accompanied by a dilatation of the brain. This fact, therefore, fits exactly the view that is being considered. The peripheral constriction, by raising general blood-pressure, dilates the brain more or less, and, what is more important, drives more blood through it. It is difficult to understand why psychical activity is always associated in this way with a peripheral con- striction unless the object of the reflex is to increase the blood- supply to the brain. Even if vasomotor fibers are subsequently shown to be present in the brain, the importance of this reflex in * Francois-Franck, "Journal de phys. et de path, gen.," 1, 724, 1889. f See Hunter, " Journal of Physiologie, " 26, 465, 1902. VASOMOTOR SUPPLY OF THE ORGANS. 561 providing a greater flow to the central organ at the time that it is in activity must still be admitted. A general irrigation, so to speak, is provided for by this means. Local vasomotors may be used to divert this flow mainly through one or another cerebral area. Vasomotor Nerves of the Head Region. — The vasomotor supply of the various parts of the head, including the mouth cavity, has been investigated by many observers. It would appear from the results of most of these investigations that the vasoconstrictor supply for the skin, including the ears, the eye, the mouth, and buccal glands, is derived mainly, if not entirely, from the sympa- thetic nervous system. These fibers arise from the spinal cord in the upper thoracic nerves, first to the fifth or sixth, emerge by the rami communicantes to the sympathetic chain, in which they pass upward and end, for the most part, in the superior cervical ganglion. From this ganglion they are distributed, by various routes, as postgan- glionic fibers. In one interesting instance at least the constrictor fibers for the head take a somewhat different course. It was shown by Schiff, long ago, that in the rabbit the ear receives vaso- motor fibers from the auricularis magnus nerve, a branch of the third cervical nerve. Later investigations indicate (Meltzer) that the ear, in fact, receives most of its vasoconstrictor fibers by this route. Fletcher, however, has shown that these fibers do not emerge from the brain in the roots of the third cervical, but rather in the general outflow from the thoracic region. After reaching the sym- pathetic chain these particular fibers pass to the third cervical by the gray rami from the first thoracic ganglion, which communicate with a number of the cervical nerves. On the other hand, the vaso- dilator fibers for the head are supplied in part by way of the cervical sympathetic, following the same general path as the constrictors, and in part by way of the cranial nerves (seventh, ninth) and the sympathetic ganglia with which they connect. According to Langley, the outflow of the seventh nerve passes to the spheno- palatine ganglion, whence as postganglionic fibers they accompany the branches of the superior maxillary nerve and cause vasodila- tation in the membrane of the nose, soft palate, tonsils, uvula, roof of mouth, upper lips, gums, and pharynx. The fibers that emerge in the ninth pass in part directly to the tongue and in part terminate first in the otic ganglion, whence they are distributed with the branches of the inferior maxillary to the lower lips, cheeks, gums, >arotid and orbital glands. Dastre and Morat describe the vaso- lilators in the cervical sympathetic as reaching the fifth cranial nerve by communicating branches from the superior cervical ;anglion and state that they cause dilatation of the bucco-facial region, — that is, the lips, the gums, cheeks, palate, nasal mucous membrane, and the corresponding skin areas. 36 562 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. The Trunk and the Limbs. — The vasoconstrictor fibers for these regions are distributed, so far as is known, chiefly to the skin. They are all derived immediately from the sympathetic chain and ultimately from the outflow in the anterior roots of the thoracic and lumbar spinal nerves. Those for the upper limbs arise from the midthoracic region chiefly (fourth to ninth thoracic nerves), those for the lower limbs arise in the nerves of the lower thoracic and upper lumbar region (eleventh, twelfth, thirteenth thoracic [dog] and first and second lumbar). The vasodilator fibers in the nerves of the limbs have been demonstrated frequently, as already explained. Whether or not such fibers are found in the sympathetic system following the same general course as the constrictors has not been shown conclusively. The most definite work at present (Bayliss) indicates that the vasomotor effect is directly caused in some unknown way by sensory fibers arising in the posterior roots of the nerves forming the brachial and the sciatic plexus. The unsatisfactory explanations offered for this result have been re- ferred to (p. 548). The Abdominal Organs. — The stomach and intestines receive their most important supply of vasoconstrictor fibers by way of the splanchnic nerves and celiac ganglion. These fibers emerge from the cord in the lower thoracic spinal nerves, from the fifth down, and the upper lumbar nerves, and they supply the whole mesenteric circulation as far as the descending colon. According to some observers (Francois-Franck and Hallion), the mesenteric vessels receive a supply of vasodilator fibers by the same general route, and it is also stated that similar fibers reach this region through the vagus nerve. Concerning this latter statement at least further con- firmation is necessary. The pancreas has been shown to receive vasoconstrictor fibers by way of the splanchnics, and the kidney, according to Bradford, receives vasodilator as well as vasocon- strictor fibers from the same nerve. Most of the vasomotor fibers to the kidney of the dog emerge from the cord in the roots of the eleventh, twelfth, and thirteenth thoracic nerves, and those for the liver (Frangois-Franck and Hallion) come from about the same region. The vasomotor supplies of the spleen and the bladder have not as yet been investigated successfully. The Genital Organs. — Both vasoconstrictor and vasodilator fibers have been discovered for the external genital organs (penis, scrotum, clitoris, vulva). The vasoconstrictors arise in the dog from the thirteenth thoracic to the fourth lumbar nerves, pass over to the sympathetic chain, and thence reach the organs either by way of the hypogastric nerve and pelvic plexus or by way of the sacral sympathetic ganglia and their branches to the pudic nerves. The vasodilator fibers arise from the sacral spinal nerve, being the VASOMOTOR SUPPLY OF THE ORGANS. 563 best known of the sacral autonomic system. They enter the ner- vus erigens and thence reach the organs by way of the pelvic plexus. The especial importance of these fibers in the process of erection is described in the section on the physiology of the repro- ductive organs. The internal genital organs — uterus, vagina, vas deferens, seminal vesicles, etc. — receive no vasomotor fibers from the sacral autonomic system, — that is, from the nervi erigentes — but do receive a supply of constrictor fibers from the sympathetic system. These latter fibers emerge from the cord in the roots of the upper lumbar nerves and reach the organs by way of the in- ferior mesenteric ganglion and hypogastric nerve.* Vasomotor Supply of the Skeletal Muscles. — Gaskellf es- pecially has given evidence of the existence of vasomotor fibers in the muscles. He concludes, as the result of his work, that the blood- vessels of the muscles receive both vasoconstrictor and vasodilator fibers, but that the latter greatly predominate, — at least, their physiological effect is much more evident in experimental work. As proof of the presence of dilator fibers he gives such results as these: The mylohyoid muscle of the frog is thin enough to be observed directly under the microscope. When curarized and stimulated through its motor nerve the small vessels may be seen to dilate and there is an augmented flow of blood. In a dog section of the motor nerve to a muscle is followed by a greatly increased flow of blood, which, however, is only temporary and is referable to a mechanical stimulation of the dilator fibers. Direct stimulation of the severed nerve causes an increased flow of blood through the muscles, but if the muscles are first completely curarized stimulation causes, on the contrary, a decreased flow. This last result is ex- plained on the supposition that curare paralyzes the endings of the dilator fibers and thus allows the effects of the constrictors to mani- fest themselves. Since, however, Bayliss has shown or claimed (p. 548) that the dilator effect in the limbs is due to the antidromic action of sensory fibers, it is evident that this important question needs reinvestigation. Various physiologists have shown that muscular activity is accompanied by an increase in the blood-flow through the muscle, as we should expect, but it remains uncertain whether this result is brought about solely by an increased activity of the heart or by the combined effect of vasodilatation and in- crease in heart- work. Kaufmann J takes this latter view in con- sequence of some interesting results obtained upon horses. He measured the blood-flow through the masseter muscle and the * For the bibliography of the vasomotor supply to the various organs see, Langley, " Ergebnisse der Physiologie, " vol ii, part II, p. 820, 1903. fGaskell, " Journal of Physiology," 1, 262, 1878-79. X Kaufmann, " Archives de physiologie normale et pathologique, " 1892, pp. 279 and 495. 564 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. elevator of the lip in a horse in which the muscles were exercised normally by the act of eating. The blood-flow was increased as much as five times over that observed during rest, and that this increase was due in part at least to a local dilatation seems to be proved by the fact that the blood-pressure in the artery supplying the muscle fell, while that in the vein rose. While, therefore, our experimental knowledge of the vasomotors of the muscles needs further investigation, we may provisionally accept the view ad- vocated by Gaskell, — namely, that the vasomotor supply to the muscles consists essentially of dilator fibers and that these fibers are brought into action reflexly whenever the muscles contract, thus providing an increased blood-flow in proportion to the func- tional activity. The Vasomotor Nerves to the Veins. — It is assumed in physi- ology that the vasoconstrictors and vasodilators end in the muscula- ture of the small arteries. The veins also, however, have a muscular coat, and it is possible that if this musculature were innervated from the central nervous system we should have another efficient factor in controlling the blood-flow. Mall has given very clear proof that the portal vein receives vasoconstrictor fibers from the splanchnic nerve* but this supply may be exceptional, as the portal system itself is unique. The portal vein, indeed, plays the role physiologically of an artery in regard to the liver. Roy and Sherrington f give some evidence for the existence of venomotor nerves to the large veins of the neck, and Thompson, as also Ban- croft^ reports experiments in which it was found that stimulation of the sciatic nerve caused a visible constriction of the superficial veins of the hind limbs. The whole subject, however, of venomotor nerves has been but little investigated, and at present little or no use is made of this possible system in explaining the facts of the circulation. THE CIRCULATION OF THE LYMPH. The direction of flow of the lymph is from the tissues toward the large lymphatic trunks, the thoracic and the right lymphatic duct. The flow is maintained in this direction mainly by a difference in pressure at the two ends. At the opening of the large trunks into the veins the pressure is very low; in the vein, in fact, it may be zero or even negative. The opening between the lymph vessel and the vein is protected by a valve which opens toward the vein, and the lymph, therefore, will flow into the vein as long as the pres- sure in the latter is lower than that in the lymphatic duct. At the other ex- tremity of the system, in the tissue spaces to which the lymphatic capillaries are distributed, the pressure, on the contrary, is high. Its exact amount is not known, but, since the pressure in the blood capillaries is equal to 40-60 mms. Hg., the pressure in the liquid of the surrounding tissues must also be considerable. The tissues are, in fact, in a condition of turgidity owing to * Mall, " Archiv f . Physiologie, " 1892, p. 409. f Roy and Sherrington, "Journal of Physiology," 11, 85, 1890. % Bancroft, "American Journal of Physiology," 1, 477, 1898. VASOMOTOR SUPPLY OF THE ORGANS. 565 the pressure of the lymph in the tissue-spaces. This difference in pressure at the two ends of the lymphatic system is the main constant factor in mov- ing the lymph. It is obvious that in the long run it is dependent upon the pressure within the blood-vessels and therefore upon the force of the heart beat. The contractions of the heart supply the energy, not only for the move- ment of the blood, but also for the much slower movement of the lymph. The circulation of the lymph is aided, however, by many accessory factors. In some animals there are genuine lymph hearts upon the course of the vessels, — that is, pulsatile expansions of the lymph vessels whose force of beat, con- trolled by valves, is directly applied to moving the lymph. No such structures are found in the mammalia, but according to some observers the large re- ceptacle at the beginning of the thoracic duct, receptaculum chyli may undergo contractions, and is, besides, under the influence of motor and inhibitory nerves. Such movements, if they occur, must be equivalent to the action of a lymph heart in their influence upon the flow of lymph. The flow of lymph or chyle in the intestinal area is also, without doubt, greatly assisted by the peristaltic and especially by the pendular contractions of the musculature of the intestines. The volume of the lymph in this region is especially large and the lymph capillaries and veins are provided with valves. Rhythmical contractions of the musculature of the intestine must squeeze the lymph toward the thoracic duct, acting like a local pump to accelerate the flow of lymph. A similar influence is exerted by the contractions of the skeletal muscles. The compression exerted by the shortened fibers squeezes the lymph vessels and, on account of the valves present, forces the lymph onward toward the larger ducts. The flow of lymph from the resting muscles — the arms and legs, for instance — is normally small in quantity, but during muscular exercise and massage it is obviously increased. This increase may be observed in experimental work by placing a cannula in the thoracic duct. Active or passive movements of the limbs under these conditions will cause a noticeable increase in the outflow from the duct. Still another factor which exercises an influence upon the flow of lymph is found in the respiratory move- ments of the thorax. At each inspiration the pressure within the thorax is diminished (increase of negative pressure), and this factor influences the lymph flow in several ways : By increasing the flow of blood through the large veins at the edge of the thorax, jugulars and subclavians, it doubtless aspirates lymph from the thoracic and right lymphatic ducts into these veins. More- over, by lowering the pressure upon the intrathoracic portion of the thoracic duct it also aspirates the lymph from the abdominal portion of this vessel. When we place a cannula in the thoracic duct and measure the outflow directly it is found to be exceedingly slow and variable. Older measure- ments (Weiss) indicate that it has a velocity in the duct in the neck of about 4 mms. per second, but this velocity changes naturally with the conditions influencing the production of lymph in the tissues. Heidenhain estimates that for a dog weighing 10 kgms. the total outflow from the thoracic duct in 24 hours is equal to 640 c.c. Munk and Rosenstein, from observations upon a case with a lymph fistula, estimated that in man the flow may be equal to 50 to 100 or 120 c.c. per hour. SECTION VI. PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. Historical. — The term respiration as usually employed in physiology refers to the process of gaseous exchange between an organism and its environment. This exchange consists essentially in the absorption of oxygen by the living matter and the elimination of carbon dioxid. It is one of the generalizations of physiology that all living matter, with the exception perhaps of the anaerobic organisms, requires oxygen for its vital processes, — that is, its characteristic metabolism. On the other hand, one of the universal end-products of this metabolism is carbon dioxid. Hence, respira- tion in some form is one great characteristic of living things. In the simplest animals and plants, the unicellular organisms, the exchange between the air (or water) and the organism takes place directly, but in the more complex animals some form of respiratory apparatus is developed whose function consists either in bringing the air or oxygen-laden water to the constituent cells, as in the air tubes of the insects, or in bringing the circulating blood into contact with the air or water, as in the case of animals provided with lungs or gills. In man and the air-breathing vertebrates the latter device is employed and one may distinguish in such animals between internal and external respiration. By the latter term is meant the gaseous exchange, absorption of oxygen and elimination of carbon dioxid, that takes place in the lungs between the blood in the pul- monary capillaries and the air in the alveoli. By internal respira- tion is meant the similar exchange that takes place in the systemic capillaries between the blood and the tissue elements. All of this exchange is, so to speak, secondary, since the essential process consists in the history of the oxygen after it is absorbed into the tissues, — that is, the part taken by the oxygen in the metabolism of living matter. This process, however, is a part of the subject of nutrition. The food absorbed from the digestive organs and the oxygen taken from the blood have a common history, or at least their reactions are indissolubly connected after they come within the field of influence of the living molecules. This side of the func- tion of the oxygen may be considered, therefore, more appropriately in the section on nutrition. In the present section attention will be directed to the beautiful means that have been adapted to the pur- 566 HISTORICAL. 567 pose of supplying the tissues with oxygen and of removing the carbon dioxid. The true understanding of the object of the act of respiration we owe to Lavoisier, the discoverer of oxygen. In his paper published in 1777, entitled "Experiments on the Respiration of Animals and on the Changes which the Air Undergoes in Passing through the Lungs/' he laid the foundations of our present knowledge, and in subsequent work he developed a conception of the nature of physiological oxidations which has dominated the physiological theories of nutrition up to the present time. The discovery of the physiological meaning of respiration and. the function of the lungs constitutes the most interesting part of the history of physiology. All the great physiologists of past ages contributed their part to the story, and as we look back we can count distinctly the different steps made toward the truth as we understand it to-day. The history of this subject is not only most instructive in demonstrating the triumphant although slow progress of scientific investigation, but it illustrates well also the intimate interrelations of physiology with the sister sciences of chemistry and physics and the great value of the experimental method. The theory of respiration held in each century was formulated to explain, as far as possible, the facts that were known, and as we look back from our vantage point it is most impressive to realize how well-known phenomena, imperfectly understood, were apparently explained by theories which we now know to be incorrect. Without doubt, many of the explanations accepted to-day will in later times be found to rest upon a similar incomplete knowledge. Each generation must do the best it can with the knowledge of its times. The history of respiration, the successive steps in its progress may be summarized in a few words. Aristotle thought that the main function of respiration is to regulate the heat of the body, which was supposed to be produced in the heart; hence the increased respira- tions after muscular exercise when the body-heat is increased. At the same time he believed, with the philosophers of his times, that the body receives something from the air that is necessary to life, a subtle something that he designated as the " pneuma." Praxagoras taught that blood is contained only in the veins, and that the ar- teries are filled with a gaseous substance, the "pneuma" derived from the air, an unfortunate error that prevailed in medicine for several centuries. The two celebrated anatomists and physiologists of the Alexandrian school, Herophilus and Erasistratus, distin- guished two kinds of pneuma, the vital spirits, which are made or extracted from the air in the lungs and whose production consti- tutes the chief function of respiration, and the animal spirits, elabo- rated in the brain from the vital spirits and responsible for the 568 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. functions of motion and sensation. Galen (131 A. D.) demonstrated that the arteries as well as the veins contain blood, but still believed that the chief function of the respiratory movements is to furnish pneuma or vital spirits to the heart. This great physiologist noticed also that the air is necessary for combustion as it is for life, and stated his belief that the explanation of one of these acts would be also an explanation of the other. This thought seems to have been accepted by all the physiologists of subsequent times, but it required over sixteen hundred years of investigation before a satis- factory solution was reached. Galen recognized, moreover, that not only does the blood take something of essential importance from the air, — namely, vital spirits, — but it also gives off something to the air that is injurious to the body, a something which he compared to the smoke of combustion and designated as the " fuliginous vapor. " If we substitute oxygen for vital spirits and carbon dioxid for fuliginous vapor we realize that the essential problem of respiration was already clearly formulated, but could not make further advance until chemical knowledge was more fully developed. Such is the case with some of our physiological problems to-day. Galen also explained satisfactorily the respiratory movements, the action of the muscles of inspiration and expiration, thus destroying the older erroneous theories that the expansion and contraction of the lungs are due to processes of heating and cooling. Galen's physiology held undisputed sway until the seventeenth century. At that time there arose a school of physiologists, the iatromechanists, who proposed to explain all vital phenomena upon known mechanical principles, — the laws of physics and chemistry. For the mystical view of vital spirits they proposed to substitute a more rational and concrete theory. The blood in the lungs becomes red simply because it is minutely subdivided and shaken, just as a tube of blood becomes red when violently agitated. Thus an effort to be more scientific, to use the exact knowledge of physics, led to the adoption of views which we now know were far more erroneous than the ancient and intrinsically correct conception that the blood receives something from the air in the lungs. In the seventeenth century, however, began those discoveries in chemistry and physiology which eventually led to our present knowledge. Van Helmont (1577-1644) discovered that in the burning of charcoal, the fermentation of wine, and the action of vinegar on chalk a special gas is produced which he called gas sylvestre and which we call carbon dioxid. Robert Boyle (1627- 1691) published a most interesting series of experiments made with the aid of the recently discovered air-pump which demonstrated the correctness of the view held by Galen that the air contains some- thing necessary for life and for combustion. He showed, moreover, HISTORICAL. 569 that air that had been repeatedly inspired was no longer capable of maintaining life. Robert Hooke (1635-1703) introduced a method of artificial respiration by means of a bellows, and demon- strated by sending a continuous stream of air through the lungs that the respiratory movements of these organs are in themselves, as a mechanical process, in no wise an essential feature of respiration. John Mayow in 1688-1674 discovered that air is not a simple ele- ment, but contains a definite substance necessary to life and to combustion. He designated this substance as the nitro-aerian vapor or nitrous particles, because he believed that the same substance is present in condensed form, as it were, in common niter, having found that combustion is possible even in a vacuum in the presence of niter. In the eighteenth century, as is shown in the work of the great physiologist, Haller, the theories of respiration were in many respects in a most unsatisfactory state. The new facts that had been discovered made the old views untenable, but were not in themselves sufficient to explain clearly what actually takes place. Such periods of uncertainty and dissatisfaction are frequent enough in the history of science. In certain parts of physiology to-day we can recognize a similar state of affairs and from the history of respiration we may assume that periods of this character are necessary transitions to a fuller and more satisfactory knowledge. In 1757 Joseph Black rediscovered carbon dioxid, calling it fixed air, and showed that it is present in expired air. A little later Priestly discovered and isolated oxygen and nitrogen; but, under the influence of an erroneous view of combustion that had been advanced by Stahl, was unable to give his discoveries a clear and satisfactory application. The final step in this progress was made by the wonderful work of Lavoisier between the years 1771 and 1780. He made correct analyses of air and of carbon dioxid, he explained combustion as an oxidation with the formation of C02 and H20, he showed that in respiration the same process occurs and that the blood takes oxygen from the air and gives back to it in expiration the carbon dioxid and water formed by combustion within the body. He gave us the essential facts in the modern theories of respiration and physiological oxidations. After Lavoisier the chief positive advances that have been made have been in reference to the condition of the gases in the blood. By means of the gas-pump Magnus (1837) obtained these gases quantitatively and thus procured data which, as Liebig showed, demonstrate that the oxygen is held in the blood, not in simple solution, but in some form of chemical combination, probably with the red corpuscles. Finally it was shown by Stokes and Hoppe-Seyler that the oxygen is held in definite chemical com- 570 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. bination with the hemoglobin. The nature of the combination of the carbon dioxid in the blood is not yet entirely understood, while the actual nature of physiological oxidations — that is, the part taken by the oxygen in the chemical reactions of living matter- is one of the great problems of nutrition which may need many years for solution. CHAPTER XXXIV. THE ORGANS OF EXTERNAL RESPIRATION AND THE RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. Anatomical Considerations. — Some of the anatomical ar- rangements in the lungs which have an immediate physiological interest may be recalled briefly. The structure of the trachea and bronchi is admirably adapted to their functions as air tubes, in that the walls possess flexibility combined with rigidity. The lining of ciliated epithelium throughout the air passages is of importance, primarily it may be assumed, in removing mucus and foreign material from these passages. The smaller bronchi possess a dis- tinct muscular layer, and, as we shall see, this musculature is under the control of a special set of nerve fibers through whose reflex activity the capacity and resistance of the bronchial system may be modified. The smallest bronchioles are expanded into a system of membranous air cells, and in the walls of these thin sacs the capil- laries of the pulmonary artery are distributed. The great efficiency of this apparatus is evident when one recalls that every one of the infinite number of red corpuscles is exposed separately to the air in the air cells, so that although the time of transit is brief the entire amount of hemoglobin is nearly completely saturated with oxygen. Each lung is enveloped in its own pleural sac. The space between the parietal and the visceral layer of each sac is the so-called pleural cavity, but it must be borne in mind that under all normal conditions this cavity is only potential, — that is, the parietal and visceral layers are everywhere in contact with each other. Under pathological or accidental conditions air or exudations may enter this space and form an actual cavity. Along the mid-line of the body and around the roots of the lungs we have the mediastinal spaces lying between the pleural sacs of the two sides, but entirely filled with the various thoracic viscera, such as the heart, aorta and its branches, pulmonary artery and veins, venae cavae, azygos vein, trachea, esophagus, thoracic duct, various nerves, and lymph EXTERNAL RESPIRATION AND RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 571 glands. All these organs, therefore, lie outside the lungs. A schematic view of these relations is represented in Fig. 234. The Thorax as a Closed Cavity. — The thorax is a cavity entirely shut off from the outside and from the abdominal cavity. In this cavity lie the lungs and the various viscera enumerated above. The lungs may be considered as two large, membranous sacs, as represented in Fig. 234, the interior of which communicates freely with the outside air through the trachea, glottis, etc., while the outside of the sacs is protected from atmospheric pressure by the walls of the chest. The atmospheric pressure on the interior surfaces of the lungs expands these structures under normal con- ditions until they fill the en- tire thoracic cavity not occu- pied by other organs. How- ever the size of the chest cavity varies, that of the lungs must change accord- ingly; so that at all times the lungs fully fill up every part of the cavity not otherwise occu- pied. If the wall of the thorax is opened at any point so as to make communication with the outside air, or, if the wall of the lung is pierced so that the air can communicate with the pleural cavity, then at once the lungs shrink in size, since the atmospheric pressure is then equalized on the outside and the inside of the sacs. We may consider, therefore, that the thoracic cavity is much larger than the lungs, and that the latter are blown out to fill this cavity by the atmospheric pressure on the inside. The Normal Position of the Thorax — Inspiration and Expira- tion.— During life the size of the thorax is continually changing with the respiratory movements. But the size and position taken at the end of a normal expiration may be regarded as the normal position of the thorax ; that is, its position when all of the muscles of respira- tion are at rest, and substantially, therefore, the position of the thorax in the cadaver. Starting from this position, any enlarge- ment of the thorax constitutes an active inspiration, the result of which will be to draw more air into the lungs ; while starting from the normal position any diminution in the size of the thorax Fig. 234. — Schema to indicate the re- lations of the parietal and visceral layers of the pleural sacs, and the position of the me- diastinal space: P, the potential pleural cavity in each sac; M, the mediastinal space; R.L. and L.L., the cavity of the right and the left lung, respectively; T, the trachea. The outlines of the pleura on each side are represented in dotted lines. 572 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. constitutes an active expiration, which will drive some air out of the lungs. It is evident, however, that after an active inspiration the thorax may return passively to its normal position, giving what is known as a passive expiration, — that is, an expiration not caused by muscular effort. So after an active expiration the thorax may return passively to its normal position, giving a passive inspiration. Our normal respiratory movements consist of an active inspiration followed by a passive expiration, Mechanism of the Inspiration. — The chest cavity may be enlarged and an inspiration, therefore, be produced by two methods, — namely, by a contraction of the diaphragm and by an elevation of the ribs. Contraction of the Diaphragm. — From the anatomy of the diaphragm it is evident that its fixed attachment is found in its muscular connections with the lumbar vertebra?, the ribs, and the ensiform cartilage. From these attachments the muscular sheet extends anteriorly along the walls of the thorax and then bends over to form the arch which ends in the central tendon. This latter structure is not entirely free, since it is attached to the pericar- dium of the heart ; but, relatively, it is the movable portion of the diaphragm. Speaking generally, a contraction of the dia- phragmatic muscle draws the central tendon downward toward the abdominal cavity and therefore enlarges the chest in the vertical diameter, while an increase in the thoracic cavity around the periphery of the diaphragm is caused also by the flattening of the muscular arch. Two results follow this movement: The lungs are expanded exactly in proportion as the cavity enlarges. There is, of course, at no time any space between the lungs and the dia- phragm : as the latter moves downward the lungs follow because of the excess of pressure on their interior. Although ordinarily we speak of the new air being sucked into the lungs during this move- ment, it is, of course, strictly speaking, forced in by the pressure of the outside atmosphere. On the other hand, the descent of the dia- phragm raises the pressure in the abdominal cavity. This cavity is entirely full of viscera and for mechanical purposes may be regarded as being full of liquid. The rise of pressure is transmitted throughout the abdomen and causes the abdominal wall to protrude. Inspiration caused by a contraction of the diaphragm is therefore spoken of either as diaphragmatic respiration or as abdominal respiration, the latter term having reference to the visible effect on the abdominal walls. In strong contractions of the diaphragm the heart also is pulled downward, and if the movement is forced the lower ribs may be pulled inward to some extent. This last effect would diminish the size of the thorax and therefore tend to antagonize the inspira- tory action of the diaphragm, and other muscles are apparently EXTERNAL RESPIRATION AND RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 573 Fig. 235.- Sixth dorsal vertebra and rib. — (Reichert.) brought into play to prevent this result. As stated below, the quadratus lumborum and the serratus posticus inferior may have this function of fixating the lower ribs in violent inspirations. The diaphragmatic muscle is innervated on each side by the corresponding phrenic nerve. This nerve arises in the neck from the fourth and fifth cervical spinal nerves, and passes downward in the chest in the mediastinal space, lying close to the heart in part of its course. Section of this nerve paralyzes, of course, the diaphragm on the cor- responding side. Elevation of the Ribs. — As a necessary result of the structure of the bony thorax, every elevation of the ribs must cause an enlarge- ment of the thoracic cavity in the dorsoventral and the lateral diam- eters. We are justified in saying that every muscle whose contrac- tion causes an elevation of the ribs is an inspiratory muscle. This result is due, in the first place, to the slant of the ribs. Each rib is attached to the spinal column at two points : the head to the body of the vertebra and the tubercle to the transverse process. The up-and- down movements of the ribs may be re- garded as rotations around an axis joining these two points, — that is, each point in the rib as it moves up or down describes a circle around this axis (see Fig. 235). If our ribs were set upon the vertebral col- umn so that the plane of the rib formed a right angle with the column, then every movement of the rib up or down would decrease the size of the thorax and there- fore cause an expiration. As a matter of fact, however, the ribs slant downward, so that if elevated the sternal end is car- ried farther away from the sternum and the chest is enlarged in the dorsoventral Moreover, as the rib moves upward there Fig. 236.— Diagram to il- lustrate the effect of the slant of the ribs: S, The spinal col- umn; a, the position of the rib in normal expiration; (a') its position (exaggerated) in inspiration (the distance be- tween the spinal column and the sternum (st.), the antero- posterior or dorsoventral di- ameter of the chest is in- creased) . Any movement from the position a' would cause an expiration. direction (see Fig. 236). 574 . PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. - fm^s I is^ifi obvious enlargement of the chest in the lateral diameter. result may be referred to two causes : In the first place, the of the rotation of the ribs, — that is, the line joining the head id the tubercle of the rib is inclined downward so that the plane rotation, which is, of course, at right angles to this axis, will be inclined outward. As the rib is moved upward, therefore, it must also move outward. Secondly the cartilaginous ends of the ribs are fixed at the sternum so that as they move upward and outward they will be twisted or everted somewhat in the middle, with a torsion of the cartilaginous ends. The Muscles of Inspiration. — In addition to the diaphragm, all muscles attached to the thorax whose contraction causes an elevation of the ribs must be classed as inspiratory muscles. In regard to this latter group the action of some of them is either evident from their anatomical attachments, or the muscles may be stimulated directly and the effect of their contraction noted. In other cases, however, it is necessary to make use of the method first suggested by Newell Martin, — namely, to determine whether the contraction of the muscle in respiration occurs simultaneously with that of the diaphragm or alternately with it. In the former case it is inspiratory, in the latter expiratory. The following mus- cles may be classed as inspiratory : Levatores costarum. They arise from the transverse process of the seventh cervical and first to eleventh thoracic vertebrae and are inserted into the next- rib or the second rib below. Intercostales externi muscles. They lie in the inter- costal spaces extending from the lower edge of one rib to the upper edge of the rib below ; they slant downward and toward the mid-line. These muscles have been assigned different functions by different authors, but the experiments made by Hough,* using the method of Martin described above, show that they are inspiratory. It was found that in the dog they contract synchronously with the diaphragm. The same authors find that the intercartilaginous portions of the internal intercostals are also inspiratory. The scaleni — anterior, medius, and posterior — arise from the transverse processes of the cervical vertebrae and are inserted into the first and second ribs. M. sterno-cleido-mastoideus extends from the mastoid process to the sternum and sternal extremity of the clavicle. M. pectoralis minor extends from the coracoid process of the scapula to the anterior surface of the second to the fifth rib. M. serratus posticus superior extends from the spinous processes of the lower cervical and upper dorsal vertebrae to the second to fifth rib. The Muscles of Expiration. — Expiration — that is, diminution * Hough, "Studies from the Biological Laboratory, John Hopkins University," 5, 91, 1893, and Bergendal and Bergman, " Skandinavisches Archiv f. Physiologie, " 7, 178, 1896. EXTERNAL RESPIRATION AND RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. $75 O ' in size of the thorax — may also be produced in two ways: Fhtet,^ by forcing the diaphragm farther into the thoracic cavity. This ' result is obtained, not by any direct action of the diaphragm, b\t by contracting the muscular walls of the abdomen, the external am internal oblique, the rectus, and the transversus. The contraction' of these muscles, which form what has been called the abdominal press, raises the pressure in the abdomen and this, acting upon the under surface of the diaphragm, forces it up into the thorax, pro- vided the glottis is open. If the glottis is kept closed firmly the increased abdominal pressure is felt mainly upon the pelvic organs, and this process is used in micturition, defecation, and parturition. Second, by depressing the ribs. The muscles which may be sup- posed to exert this action are as follows: M. inter costales interni. The expiratory action of these muscles, so far as the interosseous • portion is concerned, was first definitely shown by Martin, who proved that when they contract they act alternately with the dia- phragm . * M . triangularis sterni or t he m . transversus thoracis is found on the interior of the thorax on the anterior wall. Its fibers pass from the sternum, running upward and outward, to be inserted into the third to sixth rib. The expiratory action of this muscle was demonstrated by Hough according to the method of Martin. M. iliocostalis lumborum. The anatomical attachments of this muscle are such as would enable it to depress the ribs; but its functional activity in expiration has not been demonstrated. The m. serratus posticus inferior and m. quadratus lumborum are both placed anatomically, especially the former, so that their contractions serve to depress the ribs. It has been suggested, however, that they may act in forced inspirations so as to antagonize the ten- dency of the diaphragm to pull the lower ribs inward. Whether they really act with the diaphragm or alternately with it can only be determined by actual experiment. So far as the author knows, such experiments have not been made. Quiet and Forced Respiratory Movements; Eupnea and Dyspnea. — Our respiratory movements vary much in amplitude, and the muscles actually involved differ naturally with the extent of the movement. In general, we distinguish two different forms of breathing movements. The ordinary quiet respirations, made without obvious effort, form a condition of respiration designated as eupnea. Difficult or labored breathing is known as dyspnea. It is impossible to draw a sharp line between the two. There are many degrees of dyspnea, and doubtless in quiet breathing the amplitude of the movements may vary considerably before they become distinctly dyspneic. In all conditions of eupnea the chief point to bear in mind is that the expiration is entirely passive. * Martin and Hartwell, " Journal of Physiology, " 2, 24, 1879. 576 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. The inspiration in man is made by the diaphragm alone or by the diaphragm together with some action of the levatores costarum and the external intercostals. At the end of the inspiration the ribs and diaphragm are brought back to the normal position by purely physical forces, — the elasticity of the distended abdominal wall, the elasticity of the expanded lungs, the weight and torsion of the ribs, etc. As soon as the breathing movements become at all forced the action of the above-named inspiratory muscles is in- creased in intensity, and the other inspiratory muscles, all elevators of the ribs, come into play. Quiet breathing in man at least is mainly diaphragmatic or abdominal, while dyspneic breathing is characterized by a greater action of the elevators of the ribs. When dyspnea reaches a certain stage the expiration also becomes active or forced. The expiratory act is hastened by a contraction of the abdominal muscles or of the depressors of the ribs, and indeed the action of these muscles may compress the chest beyond its normal position, so that the expiration is followed by a passive inspiration which brings the chest to its normal position before the next active inspiration begins. Costal and Abdominal Types of Respiration. — These two types of respiration are based upon the character of the inspiratory movement. An inspiration in which the movement of the abdomen, due to contraction of the diaphragm, is the chief or only feature belongs to the abdominal type. An inspiration in which the eleva- tion of the ribs is a noticeable factor belongs to the costal type. Hutchinson, who introduced this nomenclature,* laid emphasis chiefly upon the order of the movements. In the abdominal type the abdomen bulges outward first, and this is followed by a movement of the thorax; the movement spreads from the abdomen to the thorax, and, " like a wave, is lost over the thoracic region." In costal breathing the upper ribs move first and the abdomen second. The terms are meant to apply chiefly to human respiration and have aroused interest in connection with the fact that in quiet breathing in the erect posture the respiration of man belongs to the abdominal type and that of woman to the costal type. It has been a question whether this difference is a genuine sexual distinction or depends simply upon differences in dress. Hutchinson inclined to the view that it forms what we should call a secondary sexual characteristic, and that its physio- logical value for woman lies in the fact that provision is thus made, as it were, against the period of pregnancy. He states that in twenty-four young girls examined between the ages of eleven and fourteen the costal type was present, although none of them had * See Hutchinson, article on "Thorax," Todd's "Cyclopaedia of Anat- omy and Physiology, " 1849. EXTERNAL RESPIRATION AND RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 577 worn tight dress. Later observers, however (Mays, Kellogg, and others), state that Indian and Chinese women who have not worn tight dress exhibit the abdominal type, and the same statement is made regarding civilized white women who habitually wear loose clothing. It would appear, therefore, that the assumption of the costal type by women in general is due to the hindrance offered by the clothing to the movements of the abdomen. From an exami- nation of four hundred and seven cases Fitz * concludes that when the restricting effect of dress is removed there is little or no differ- ence in the type of respiration in the two sexes. The natural type is one in which " the movement is fairly equally balanced between chest and abdomen, the abdominal being somewhat in excess." When the respiration becomes dyspneic it takes on a distinctly costal type, and Fitz and others have shown that for an equal in- crease in girth the thoracic movements cause a greater enlargement of the lungs. Accessory Respiratory Movements.— In addition to the mus- cles whose action directly enlarges or diminishes the capacity of the thorax certain other muscles connected with the air passages con- tract rhythmically with the inspirations, and may be designated properly as accessory muscles of inspiration. The muscles es- pecially concerned are those controlling the size of the glottis and the opening of the external nares. At each inspiration the elevators of the wings of the nose come into play. This movement occurs in normal breathing in many animals, such as the rabbit and horse, and in some men, while in dyspneic breathing it is invariably present. The useful result of the movement is to reduce the resis- tance to the inflow of air. So in many animals the glottis is dilated at each inspiration by the contraction of the posterior crico-aryte- noicl muscles, and in man also this movement is evident when the breathing is at all forced. The useful result in this case also is a reduction in the resistance offered to the inflow of air. The Registration of the Rate and Amplitude of the Respira- tory Movements. — Many methods are employed to register the rate or amplitude of the respiratory movements. Upon man the amplitude may be measured directly by a tape placed at different levels to ascertain the increase in girth, or it may be recorded by some form of lever or tambour applied to the chest or abdomen. A convenient instrument for this purpose is the pneumograph described by Marey which is illustrated and described in Fig. 237. In animal experimentation the various methods that are employed may be classified under four heads: (1) Methods in which the change in circumference or diameter of the chest or abdomen is recorded. (2) Methods in which the change of pressure in the air * Fitz, "Journal of Experimental Medicine/' 1, 1896. 37 578 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. passages is recorded. In these methods a tube may be inserted into one of the nostrils for instance, and then connected to a tambour the lever of which makes its record on a kymographion, or if the animal is tracheotomized a side tube upon the tracheal cannula may be connected to a tambour. This method indicates well the rate of movement and the relative amplitude, but has the defect that it Fig. 237. — Figure of Marey's pneumograph. — (Verdin.) The instrument consists of a tambour (t), mounted on a flexible metal plate (p). By means of the bands c and c the metal plate is tied to the chest. Any increase or decrease in the size of the chest will then affect the tambour by the lever arrangement shown in the figure. These changes in the tambour are transmitted through the tube r as pressure changes in the contained air to a second tambour (not shown in the figure) which records them upon a smoked drum. does not record the pause, if any, at the end of inspiration or ex- piration. A modification of this method that permits an accurate record of the amplitude and duration of the movements consists in connecting the trachea or nostrils with a large bottle of air. The animal breathes into and out of the bottle, and the corresponding Fig. 238. — Curve of normal respiratory movements. — (Marey.) Curve A, full line, represents the movements when the respiration is entirely normal. Downstroke, inspira- tion; upstroke, expiration. CurveO, dotted line, represents the increased amplitude of the movements, slight dyspnea, caused by breathing through a narrow tube. variations in pressure are recorded by a tambour also connected with the interior of the bottle. (3) Methods in which the change of pressure in the thoracic cavity is recorded. This end may be reached by inserting a cannula into the thoracic wall so that its opening lies in the pleural cavity, or, more simply, a catheter or sound connected at the other end to a tambour may be passed down EXTERNAL RESPIRATION AND RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 579 the esophagus until its end lies in the intrathoracic portion. Variations in pressure in the mediastinal space synchronous with the respiratory movements affect the esophagus and through it the sound. (4) Methods in which the movements of the dia- phragm are recorded either by a tambour or lever thrust between the diaphragm and liver, or by hooks attached directly to muscular slips of the diaphragm. Registration of the movements in man during quiet breathing give us such a record as is seen in Fig. 238. It will be seen that the inspiration (descend- ing limb) is followed at once by an expiration, as we should expect, since, as soon as the inspiratory muscles cease to act, the physical factors mentioned above at once tend to bring the chest back to its normal position. The expiration (ascending limb) is at first rapid and toward the end very gradual, so that there is al- most a condition of rest, — an expira- tory pause. The Volumes of Air Respired and the Capacity of the Lungs. — The volume of air respired varies, of course, with the extent of the move- ments and the size of the individual. This volume may be determined readily in any given case by means of a spirometer, — a form of gasometer adapted to this purpose. The con- struction of this apparatus is repre- sented in Fig. 239. It consists of a graduated cylinder (A) and a receiver (B) filled with water. The cylinder A is counterbalanced by a weight (g) so as to move up and down in the water of B with the least possible re- sistance. The tube C passes through the wall of B and ends in the interior of A above the level of the water. The free end of this tube is connected with the mouth or nose. When one breathes through this tube the expired air passes into A, which rises from the water to receive it. If A is graduated the amount of air breathed out may be measured directly. The following terms are used: Vital capacity. By vital capacity is meant the quantity of air that can be breathed Eig. 239. — Wintrich's modifi- cation of Hutchinson's spirometer. — (Reichert.) 580 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. out by the deepest possible expiration after making the deepest possible inspiration. It gives a rough measure of lung capacity, and is used in gymnasiums and physical examinations for this pur- pose. The actual amount varies with the individual; an average figure for the adult man is 3700 c.c. Tidal air. By this term is meant the amount of air breathed out in a normal quiet expiration. A similar amount is breathed in, of course, in the previous inspira- tion, and the term tidal air designates the amount of air that flows in and out of the lungs with each quiet respiratory movement. Here, again, there are individual variations. The average figure for the adult man is 500 c.c. The complemental air. This term designates the amount of air that can be breathed in over and above the tidal air by the deepest possible inspiration. It is estimated at 1600 c.c. The supplemental air. By this term is meant the amount of air that can be breathed out, after a quiet expiration, by the most forcible expiration. It is equal also to 1600 c.c. It is evident that the complemental air plus the supplemental air plus the tidal air constitute the vital capacity. The residual air. After the most forcible expiration the lungs are far from being entirely collapsed. The volume of air that remains behind, after the sup- plemental air has been driven out, is known as the residual air. The amount of this air has been estimated directly on the cadaver (Hermann). The thorax was first pressed into a position of forced expiration; the trachea was then ligated, the chest opened, the lungs removed and their volume estimated by the amount of water dis- placed when they were immersed. The average result from such estimations was, in round numbers, 1000 c.c. Under conditions of normal breathing the reserve supply of air in the lungs is equal to the residual air plus the supplemental air, — that is, 2600 c.c. Mini- mal air. When the thorax is opened the lungs collapse, driving out the supplemental and residual air, but not quite completely. Before the air cells are entirely emptied the small bronchi leading to them collapse and their walls adhere with sufficient force to entrap a little air in the alveoli. It is on this account that the excised lungs float in water and are designated as lights by the butcher. The small amount of air caught in this way is designated as the minimal air. In the fetus before birth the lungs are entirely solid, but after birth, if respirations are made, the lungs do not collapse completely on account of the capture of the minimal air. Whether or not the lungs will float has constituted, therefore, one of the facts used in medicolegal cases to determine if a child was stillborn. The lungs during life may, under certain conditions, again become in parts entirely solid. If any of the alveoli become completely shut off from the trachea by an accident or by pathological conditions the air caught in them may be completely absorbed, after a certain interval, by the circulating blood. EXTERNAL RESPIRATION AND RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 581 The Size of the Bronchial Tree and the Ventilation of the Lungs. — Since the reserve supply of air in the lungs may amount to 2600 c.c., while the new air breathed in at each inspiration amounts to only 500 c.c, it would seem at first that the alveolar air is not very efficiently renewed by a quiet inspiration. The actual amount of ventilation effected depends on the capacity of the bronchial tree. According to observations founded partly on measurements of casts of the tree and partly upon physiological determinations made by breathing air poor in oxygen, it would seem that its total capacity may be reckoned at 140 c.c* At each inspiration, therefore, at least 360 c.c of air penetrate into the alveoli, and if evenly disseminated through the lungs add about ■$■$ to the volume of each alveolus. Once in the alveoli, diffusion must tend to spread the air rapidly, and that this occurs is shown by an interesting experiment performed by Grehant. He breathed in 500 c.c. of hydrogen instead of air and then examined the amount of hydrogen breathed out in successive expirations. Only 170 c.c were recovered in the first expiration, 180 c.c in the second, 41 in the third, and 40 in the fourth. Artificial Respiration. — In laboratory experiments artificial respiration is employed frequently after the use of curare ; when it is necessary to open the chest; after cessation of respirations from overdoses of chloroform or ether, etc The method used in almost all cases is the reverse of the normal procedure, — that is, the lungs are expanded by positive pressure (pressure in excess of atmos- pheric). A bellows or blast worked by hand or machinery is con- nected with the trachea and the lungs are dilated by rhythmical strokes. Provision is made for the escape of expired air by the use of valves or by a side hole in the tracheal cannula. Numerous forms of respiration pumps have been devised for this purpose. In cases of suspended respirations in human beings from drown- ing, electrical shocks, pressure upon the medulla, etc., it is necessary to use artificial respiration in order to restore normal breathing. Bellows naturally can not be used in such cases. Some method must be employed to expand and contract the chest alternately, and several different ways have been devised. The Marshall Hall method consists in placing the subject face down and rolling the body from this to a lateral position, making some pressure upon the back while in the prone position. The Sylvester method, which is frequently used, consists in raising the arms above the head and then bringing them down against the sides of the chest so as to compress the latter. The Howard method consists in simply com- pressing the lower part of the chest while the subject is in a supine position. Schaefer, who has recently compared these different * See Loewy, " Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 58, 416. 582 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. methods, suggests one of his own, which seems to be effective, saves labor, and is less injurious to the subject.* He describes it as fol- lows: "It consists in laying the subject in the prone posture, preferably on the ground, with a thick folded garment underneath the chest and epigastrium. The operator puts himself athwart or at the side of the subject, facing his head (see Fig. 240) and places his hands on each side over the lower part of the back (lowest ribs). He then slowly throws the weight of his body forward to bear upbn Xi i WW . -,_-.X-^ — -?-7^ Fig. 240. — Shows the position to be adopted for effecting artificial respiration in cases of drowning. — (Schaefer.) his own arms, and thus presses upon the thorax of the subject and forces air out of the lungs. This being effected, he gradually re- laxes the pressure by bringing his own body up again to a more erect position, but without moving the hands." These movements are repeated quite regularly at a rate of twelve to fifteen times a minute until normal respiration begins or the possibility of its restoration is abandoned. A half-hour or more may be required before normal breathing movements start. * Schaefer, " Medico-chirurgical Transactions, " London, vol. lxxxvii, 1904. CHAPTER XXXV. THE PRESSURE CONDITIONS IN THE LUNGS AND THORAX AND THEIR INFLUENCE UPON THE CIRCULATION, In considering the pressure changes in respiration the distinction between the pressure in the thorax outside the lungs and the pres- sure within the lungs and air passages must be kept clearly in mind. The pressure in the thoracic cavity outside the lungs may be designated as the intrathoracic pressure; it is the pressure exerted upon the heart, great blood- vessels, thoracic duct, esopha- gus, etc. The pressure in the interior of the lungs and air passages may be designated as intrapulmonic pressure. The relations of the two pressures with reference to the outside atmosphere is indicated sche- matically in Fig. 241. The Intrapulmonic Pres- sure and its Variations. — The air passages and the alveoli of the lungs are in free communi- cation with the external air; consequently in every position of rest, whether at the end of inspiration or expiration, the pressure in these cavities is equal to that of the atmos- phere outside. During the act of inspiration, however, the in- trapulmonic pressure falls tem- porarily below that of the atmosphere, — that is, during the inflow of air. The extent to which the pressure falls depends naturally upon the rapidity and amplitude of the inspiratory movement and upon the size of the opening to the exterior. The narrowest portion of the air passages is the glottis; consequently the variations in pres- sure below this point are probably greater than in the pharynx or 583 . — Diagram to illustrate how the pressure of the air is exerted through the lung walls upon the heart (H) and other organs in the mediastinal space. The pres- sure on these organs (intrathoracic pressure) is equal to one atmosphere minus the amount of the opposing pressure exerted by the ex- panded lungs. 584 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. nasal cavities. If the air passages are abnormally constricted at any point the fall of pressure during inspiration will be correspond- ingly magnified in the parts below the constriction, as happens, for instance, in bronchial asthma, edema of the glottis, cold in the head, etc. Under normal conditions the fall of pressure during a quiet inspiration is not large. Donders determined it in man by connecting a water manometer with one nostril and found that it was equal to — 9 or — 10 mms. water. At the end of an inspiration, if there is a pause, the pressure within the lungs again rises, of course, to atmospheric. During expiration, on the other hand, the collapse of the chest wall takes place with sufficient rapidity to compress the air somewhat during its escape and cause a temporary rise of pres- sure. In normal expiration Donders estimated this rise as equal to 7 or 8 mms. water. The intrapulmonic pressure may vary greatly from these figures in the positive or negative direction according to the factors mentioned above, especially the intensity of the respira- tory movement and the size of the opening to the exterior. The extreme variations are obtained when the opening to the outside is entirely shut off. When an inspiration or an expiration is made with the glottis firmly closed the pressure in the lungs, of course, rises and falls with the rarefaction or compression of the contained air. A strong inspiration under such conditions may lower the pressure by 30 to 80 mms. of mercury, while a strong expiration raises the pressure by an amount equal to 60 to 100 mms. Hg. In the act of coughing we get a similar result: the strong spasmodic expirations are made with a closed glottis and consequently cause a marked rise in the intrapulmonic pressure. Such great variations in pressure have a marked influence on the heart and the circula- tion, as is explained below. Intrathoracic Pressure. — When a reference is made to the pressure within the thorax, it is the intrathoracic pressure that is meant, — that is, the pressure in the pleural cavity and mediastinal spaces. This pressure, under normal conditions, is always negative, — that is, is always less than one atmosphere. The reason for this is simply that the lungs are distended to fill the thoracic cavity, and consequently the organs, like the heart, which lie in this cavity outside the lungs, are exposed to a pressure of one atmosphere, minus the force of elastic recoil of the lungs (see Fig. 241) . The heart and other intrathoracic organs are protected from the direct pres- sure of the air by the thoracic walls; they are pressed upon, how- ever, through the lungs, but naturally the atmospheric pressure is reduced by an amount equal to the elastic force of the distended lungs. Intrathoracic pressure, in fact, may be defined as intra- pulmonic pressure minus the elastic pull of the lungs, and since under usual conditions the intrapulmonic pressure is equal to that PRESSURE CONDITIONS IN LUNGS AND THORAX. 585 of the atmosphere, the intrathoracic pressure is less than an atmosphere by an amount equal to the recoil of the lungs. The negative pressure in the thorax is, therefore, equal to the elastic force of the lungs, and is larger the more the lungs are put upon a stretch, — that is, the deeper the inspiration. The amount of this negative pressure has been measured upon both animals and men by two methods : First by Donders method of attaching a manom- eter to the trachea and then opening the thoracic walls so as to allow the atmosphere to press upon the exterior face of the lungs. In this way the elastic force of the lungs is determined, and, as explained above, this is equivalent to the negative pressure. Second, by thrusting a trocar through the thoracic wall so that its open end may lie in the pleural or mediastinal cavity, the other end being appropriately connected with a manometer. The older observers (Hutchinson) also made experiments upon freshly excised human lungs, determining their elastic force when distended by known amounts of air. The figures obtained by these different methods have shown some variations, but the following quotations give an idea of the average extent of this negative pressure. Heynsius,* making use of the figures obtained by Hutchinson, estimates that in man the negative pressure in the thorax at the end of inspiration is — 4.5 mms. Hg, while at the end of an inspiration it is equal to — 7.5 mms. Hg, — a variation during respiration, therefore, of 3 mms. Hg. That is, assuming that the atmospheric pressure is 760 mms. Hg, the conditions of pressure in the thorax and lungs at the end of inspiration and expiration are as follows: At the End of Inspiration. At the End of Expiration. Intrapulmonic pressure . . . 760 mms. Hg. 760 mms. Hg. Intrathoracic pressure .... 752.5 " " 755.5 " " Aron gives results obtained from a healthy man in whom a can- nula was connected directly with the pleural cavity. f From 36 determinations he obtained the average result that at the end of quiet inspiration the negative pressure is — 4.64 mms. Hg (755.36) and at the end of expiration — 3.02 mms. Hg (756.98), — results considerably lower than those estimated by Heynsius. It should be borne in mind, however, that these values depend upon the condition of expansion of the chest, — that is, the position of the body and the depth of inspiration. On dogs Heynsius reports as follows: At end of inspiration, — 9.4 mms. Hg; end of expiration, — 3.9 mms. On rabbits, — 4.5 mms. and — 2.5 mms. Hg. Variations of Intrathoracic Pressure with Forced and Unusual Respirations. — After the most forcible expiration, when the air- * "Archiv f . die gesammte Physiologie, " 29, 265, 1882. t Aron, quoted from Emerson, "Johns Hopkins Hospital Reports," 11, 194, 1903. 586 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. passages are open, the intrathoracic pressure is still negative by a small amount, since the lungs are still expanded beyond what might be called their normal size, — that is, their size when the pres- sure inside and outside is the same. If, however, a forced expira- tion is made with the glottis closed, as in the straining movements of defecation, parturition, etc., then naturally the intrathoracic pressure rises with the intrapulmonary pressure. The increased pressure from the compressed air in the lungs is felt upon the organs in the mediastinal spaces. The large veins especially are affected, and the flow in them is partially blocked, as is shown by the swelling of the veins in the neck outside the thorax. The maintenance of such conditions for a considerable period may seriously affect the circulation. The same general effect is obtained also in attacks of coughing, the violent spasmodic expirations with closed glottis causing a visible venous congestion in the head from the obstruction to the venous flow into the heart. Forcible inspirations, on the other hand, lower the intrathoracic pressure — that is, increase the negativity — whether the glottis is open or closed. When the glottis is freely open and a deep inspiration is made the intrathoracic pressure may fall as much as 30 mms. Hg, — that is, become equal to 730 mms. The lungs being much more expanded exert a corre- spondingly greater elastic force. If the glottis is closed during a deep inspiration then there is little actual expansion of the lungs, but the intrapulmonary pressure falls from the rarefaction of the air in the lungs, and the intrathoracic pressure, of course, falls with it. The Origin of the Negative Pressure in the Thorax.— As is evi- dent from the above explanation, the fact that the pressure in the thorax is less than one atmosphere is due in the long run to the circumstance that the lungs are smaller than the thoracic cavity which they occupy. In the fetus the lungs are solid, and completely fill the thoracic cavity, except for the part occupied by the other organs. It has been a question whether after birth the size of the thoracic cavity is suddenly and permanently increased by the first inspiratory movements, and a negative intrathoracic pressure thus produced at once. The careful experiments of Hermann* seem to have settled this point. He proved that newly-born children between the first to the fourth day show no measurable negative pressure in the thorax and at the eighth day the pressure in the thoracic cavity is less than atmospheric by an amount equal to only — 0.4 mm. Hg. The negative pressure as we find it in the adult is evidently developed gradually, and is due to the fact that the thorax increases in size more rapidly and to a greater extent than the lungs, so that to fill the cavity the lungs become more and more expanded. It follows, also, from these facts, that the new-born * Hermann, " Archiv f. d. gesammte Physiologie, " 30, 276, 1883. PRESSURE CONDITIONS IN LUNGS AND THORAX. 587 child has practically no reserve supply of air in the lungs; at each expiration the lungs are entirely emptied (except for the minimal air). The ventilation of the lung alveoli is correspondingly more perfect than in older persons. Pneumothorax. — When the pleural cavity on either side is opened by any means air enters and causes a greater or less shrinkage of the corresponding lung. This condition of air within the pleural cavity is designated as pneumothorax. It is evident that air may enter the pleural cavity in one of two general ways: By a puncture of the parietal pleura such as may be made by gunshot or stab wounds in the chest, or by a puncture of the visceral pleura, such as may occur, for example, by the rupture of a tubercle in pulmonary tuberculosis, the air in this case entering from the alveoli of the lungs. From the physical conditions involved it is evident that if the opening into the pleural cavity is kept patent then the lungs will collapse completely and eventually will become entirely solid, since the small amount of entrapped minimal air will be absorbed by the blood. The other lung, the heart, etc., will also be displaced somewhat from their normal position by the unusual pressure. If, however, the opening is closed, then the air in the pleural cavity may be absorbed completely by the circulating blood and the lung again . expand as this absorption takes place. In human beings pneumothorax occurs most frequently in conditions of disease, particularly pulmonary tuberculosis, and the air in the thorax is associated also with a liquid effusion, this combination being designated sometimes as hydropneumothorax.* The Aspiratory Action of the Thorax. — The negative pres- sure prevailing in the thoracic cavity must affect the organs in the mediastinal space. The intrathoracic portion of the esophagus, for instance, is exposed, at times of swallowing at least, to a full atmosphere of pressure on its interior, while on its exterior it is under the diminished intrathoracic pressure. This difference tends to dilate the tube and may aid in the act of swallowing. The main effect of the difference in pressure is felt, however, upon the flow of lymph and blood, especially the latter. The large veins in the neck and axilla are under the pressure of an atmosphere exerted through the skin, and the same is true for the inferior cava in the abdomen. But the superior and inferior cavae and the right auricle are under a pressure less than one atmosphere. This difference in pressure must act as a constant favoring condition to the flow of blood to the heart. The difference is markedly increased at each inspiration; so that at each such act there is an increase in the velocity and volume of the flow to the heart, — an effect which is * See Emerson, "Pneumothorax," Johns Hopkins Hospital Reports, 11, 1, 1903. 588 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION.. usually referred to as the aspiratory action of the thorax. At each inspiration blood is " sucked " from the extrathoracic into the intra- thoracic veins. So far as the inferior cava is concerned, this effect is augmented by the simultaneous increase in abdominal pressure. For as the diaphragm descends it raises the pressure in the ab- domen as it lowers the pressure in the thorax. The two fac- tors co-operate in forcing more blood from the abdominal to the thoracic portion of the cava. There should be, of course, a similar effect, but in the opposite direction, upon the flow in the arteries. Each inspiration should retard the arterial outflow from the aorta into its extrathoracic branches. As a matter of fact, this effect probably does not take place. The arteries are thick walled and are distended by a high internal pressure; so that the small change of pressure of three or four millimeters of mercury during inspiration is probably incapable of influencing the caliber of the arteries, while it has a distinct effect upon the thin-walled veins, whose internal pressure is very small. The changes in intra- thoracic pressure during respiration must affect the blood-flow also in the pulmonary circuit, the flow from the right to the left side of the heart. This effect is manifested in the so-called respiratory waves of blood-pressure which may be discussed briefly in this connection. Respiratory Waves of Blood-pressure. — When a record is taken of the blood-pressure the tracing shows waves, unless the respiratory movements are very shallow, which are synchronous with the respiratory movements (see Fig. 242). When the respiration is dyspneic the waves of pressure are very marked. To ascertain the exact relations of these variations to the phases of respiration it is necessary to make simultaneous tracings of blood-pressure and respiration movements with the recording pens properly superposed. In the dog it is found that the blood-pressure falls slightly at the beginning of inspiration, but rises during the rest of the act (Fig. 243). At the beginning of expiration the pressure continues to rise for a time and then falls during most of this phase. On the whole, therefore, the effect of inspiration, its final effect, is to cause a rise of arterial pressure, while the effect of expiration is to cause a fall. The relationship of the two curves varies in other animals, depend- ing, among other things, on the rapidity of the respirations; but, since most of the experimental work has been done upon the dog, our attention may be confined to the relationship shown by this animal. Two general explanations may be given for these respira- tory waves: First, that they are due to an activity of the vaso- constrictor center synchronous with that of the respiratory center. Second, that they are due to variations in the amount of blood sent out from the heart into the aorta, this variation, in turn, being due PRESSURE CONDITIONS IN LUNGS AND THORAX. 589 to the mechanical changes in pressure during respiration and their effect on the blood-flow, aided also by the fact that the heart beats more rapidly during inspiration. This second general point of view has been adopted in physiology, and to verify it numerous experi- ments have been made upon lungs placed in an artificial thorax in which the conditions of pressure could be varied at will.* As the out- come of this work, the following results have been accepted in expla- Fig. 242. — Respiratory waves of blood-pressure. Typical blood-pressure record as taken with a mercury manometer: Bp the blood-pressure record, shows the separate heart beats and the larger respiratory waves, each of which comprises six to seven heart beats. nation of the occurrence of the respiratory waves of blood-pressure : (1) During inspiration there is an increased flow of blood into the right auricle (aspiratory action of inspiration). (2) During inspira- tion the capacity of the blood-vessels in the lungs is increased and also the velocity of the flow ; consequently there is an increased * For discussion and literature see de Jager, "Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 20, 426, 1879, and 27, 152, 1882; also " Journal of Physiology, " 7, 130. 590 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. volume of blood flowing through the lungs during inspiration. The increased capacity of the lung capillaries during the expansion of the lungs was shown experimentally by Heger and Spehl. They opened the anterior mediastinum without wounding the pleura and proved that if the lungs are tied off at the end of inspiration they con- tain more blood than when tied off at the end of expiration. The in- creased velocity of the blood-flow through the lungs during inspira- tion is explained by the fact that the greater negative pressure affects the thin- walled pulmonary veins more than the pulmonary artery; consequently the head of pressure driving the blood through the lungs, — that is, the difference in pressure between the blood in the pulmonary artery and veins — is increased. These data explain satisfactorily the general fact regarding the respiratory waves,— namely, that during inspiration there is a rise of aortic pressure due to a greater output of blood from the heart, and during expiration. Fig. 243. — Diagram to represent the time relation between the respiratory waves of blood-pressure and the respiratory movements (dog): A represents the blood-pressure record, showing the heart-beats and the larger respiratory waves. B represents a simul- taneous record of the respiratory movements. At the beginning of inspiration there is a fall of blood-pressure, but the final and main effect is a rise. At the beginning of. expi- ration there is a rise of pressure, but the final and main effect is a fall. the reverse. To account for the subsidiary fact that at the begin- ning of inspiration the pressure falls and at the beginning of expira- tion it rises for a time two explanations are offered. De Jager refers these temporary effects to the changes in capacity of the blood- bed in the lungs. At the end of inspiration there is a certain ca- pacity of the bed; when expiration comes on, the lungs shrink, the capacity of the blood-vessels is thereby diminished, and consequently some blood is squeezed out of the lungs in the direction of least resistance, — that is, toward the left auricle. This accounts for the initial rise of pressure during expiration. At the beginning of inspi- ration, on the other hand, the sudden increase in capillary capacity in the lungs retards for a moment the flow of blood to the left auricle, and thus accounts for the temporary fall of pressure. Tigerstedt,* on the other hand, finds that shutting off the entire circulation of one lung may have little or no influence upon the pressure in the * See Tigerstedt, " Ergebnisse der Physiologie, " vol. ii, part n, 560, 1903. PRESSURE CONDITIONS IN LUNGS AND THORAX. 591 systemic circulation, and therefore doubts whether small changes in the capacity of the lung vessels can have any distinct effect on the inflow into the left auricle. He thinks that the main factor is the increased flow of blood to the right auricle during inspiration, and that this increased amount is then passed on to the left auricle and ventricle, but that this takes some little time, so that the true effect of inspiration is not felt in the aorta at the very beginning of the act. This delay may vary in different animals and may account for the fact that in some animals there is an apparent inversion of the relations to respiration, the aortic pressure falling throughout inspiration and rising during expiration. The increased rate of heart beat during inspiration varies as to its degree in different individuals. It has been shown by Fredericq that this change occurs when the chest is widely opened and the respiratory movements can have no mechanical effect upon the heart. He suggests, therefore, that the accelerated pulse during inspiration is due to an associated activity in the nerve centers of the medulla. When the inspiratory center discharges it affects the neighboring cardio-inhibitory center in the direction of inhibition, lessening its tonic activity and thereby increasing the heart rate. In artificial respiration carried out by means of a bellows — that is, by expanding the lungs with positive pressure — all the conditions of pressure in inspiration and expiration are reversed. During such an inspiration the flow of blood to the right heart, and through the lungs to the left heart, is decreased. Respiratory waves of pressure are present under such conditions, but the rela- tions of rise and fall to the phases of respiration are reversed. CHAPTER XXXVI. THE CHEMICAL AND PHYSICAL CHANGES IN THE AIR AND THE BLOOD CAUSED BY RESPIRATION. The Inspired and the Expired Air. — The inspired air, atmos- pheric air, varies in composition in different places. The essential constituents from a physiological standpoint are the oxygen, nitrogen, and carbon dioxid. The new elements — argon, krypton, etc. — have not been shown to have any physiological significance, and are included with the nitrogen. The accidental constituents of the air vary with the locality. In average figures, the composi- tion of this air is, in volume per cent.: nitrogen, 79; oxygen, 20.96; carbon dioxid, 0.04. The expired air varies in composition with the depth of the expiration and, of course, with the composition of the air inspired. Under normal conditions the expired air contains, in volume per cent. : nitrogen, 79; oxygen, 16.02; carbon dioxid, 4.38. In passing once into the lungs the air, therefore, gains 4.34 volumes of carbon dioxid to each hundred, and loses 4.94 volumes of oxygen. n. o. co2. Inspired '. 79 20.96 0.04 Expired 79 16.02 4.38 4.94 4.34 This table expresses the main fact of external respiration: the respired air loses oxygen and gains carbon dioxid and consequently the blood absorbs oxygen and eliminates carbon dioxid. It will be noted, also, that the volume of oxygen absorbed is greater than the volume of carbon dioxid given off. This discrepancy is explained by the general fact that the oxygen absorbed is used in the long run to oxidize the carbon and also the hydrogen of the body; conse- quently, while most of it is eliminated in the expired air as carbon dioxid, some of it is excreted as water. For the sake of complete- ness it may be stated that traces of hydrogen and methane are also found in the expired ai . They probably originate in the intestines from fermentation processes and are carried off in solution in the blood. Physical Changes in the Expired Air.— The expired air is warmed nearly or quite to the body temperature and is nearly saturated with water vapor. Since, as a rule, the air that we 592 CHANGES IN AIR AND BLOOD IN RESPIRATION. 593 inspire is much cooler than the body and is far from being saturated with water vapor, it is evident that the act of respiration entails upon the body a loss of heat and of water. Breathing is, in fact, one of the means by which the body temperature is regulated, although in man it is a subsidiary means. In other animals — the dog, for instance — panting is a very important aid in controlling the body heat. Heat is lost in respiration not simply in warming the air in the air passages, but also by the evaporation of water in the alveoli, the conversion of water from the liquid to the gaseous form being attended by an absorption of heat. Breathing is also one of the means by which the water contents of the body are regulated. The water that we ingest or that is formed within the body is kept within certain limits, and this regulation is effected by the secretions of urine and sweat mainly, but in part also by the constant loss of water from the blood as it passes through the lungs. The Injurious Effect of Breathing Expired Air— Ventila- tion.— It is generally recognized that in badly ventilated rooms the air acquires a disagreeable odor, perceptible especially immediately on entering, and that persons remaining under such conditions for any length of time suffer from headache, depression, and a general feeling of uncomfortableness. It has been assumed, although without sufficient proof, that these effects are due to the vitiation of the atmosphere by the expired air. When the ventilation is very imperfect and the room greatly crowded death may result, as, for instance, in the historical case of the Black Hole of Calcutta. In extreme cases of this latter kind it is most probable that several causes combine to produce a fatal result. The conditions are such as to lead to a very large increase in carbon dioxid and diminution of oxygen in the respired air, — a result which in itself will cause death; and in addition the air becomes heated to a high temperature and saturated with water vapor, both of these latter conditions prevent- ing loss of heat from the body and producing a fever temperature. Under the ordinary conditions of life poor ventilation produces its obviously evil results in rooms temporarily occupied, — schools, churches, lecture rooms, theaters, etc., — and it is important to know what is the cause, and how it may be avoided. On the basis of older work it has been assumed that there is present in the expired air a volatile organic substance which when breathed again, possibly after having undergone some further change, exerts a toxic influence. The evil effects of badly ventilated rooms have been attributed mainly to this supposed substance. Unfortunately the investigations that have been made upon this substance are not altogether conclusive.* * See Haldane and Smith, "Journal of Pathology and Bacteriology," 1, 168 and 318, 1893; Merkel, " Archiv f. Hygiene," 15,' 1, 1892. Formanek, "Archiv f. Hygiene," 38, 1, 1900. 38 594 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. It seems to be clear that, when the expired air is condensed by pass- ing it into a cooled chamber, the water thus obtained, about 100 c.c. for 2500 liters of air, is clear, odorless, and has only a minute trace of organic matter. If this liquid with or without conden- sation is injected under the skin or into the blood-vessels no evil result follows, according to the testimony of the majority of observers. But it remains possible, of course, that the substance if present may be destroyed by this method or may escape precipi- tation in the condensed water. The experiment that gives the most positive indication of the existence of an organic (basic) poison in the expired air is the following, first performed by Brown- Sequard: A series of — say, five — bottles, each of a capacity of a liter or more, are connected together in train so that air can be drawn through them by an aspirator. A live mouse is placed in each bottle, and between bottles 4 and 5 an absorption tube is ar- ranged containing sulphuric acid. Under these conditions only the mouse in bottle 1 gets fresh air, those in the successive bottles get more and more impure air, while in bottle 5 this air is purified to the extent of removing the organic matter by passing it through sul- phuric acid. The result of such an experiment as described by some observers is that the mouse in bottle 4 dies after a certain num- ber of hours, the one in bottle 3 later, while those in the first and last bottles show no injurious effects. The obvious conclusion is that death in such cases is due to some organic toxic substance, and not to a mere increase of carbon dioxid, chemical analysis showing that this latter substance does not accumulate sufficiently under these conditions to cause a fatal result. Some other observers have failed to get this effect, but even assuming it to be correct it will be noted that the experiment gives no proof that the organic substance in question is excreted in the expired air. Indeed, the seemingly very careful experiments of Formanek make it probable that in these experiments the toxic substance is ammonia or an ammonia compound, which is not given off from the lungs, but from the decom- position of the urine and feces in the cage. When this latter source of contamination is removed the expired air is practically free from ammonia and without injurious effect. The expired air therefore, according to work of this character, contains no organic poison which can be regarded as a product of respiration. Some observers (Hermann, Haldane, and Smith) have made careful experiments upon men which also seem to throw much doubt upon the existence of such a substance. Individuals kept in a confined space for a number of hours give no symptoms of evil effects except when the accumulation of the carbon dioxid has reached a concentration of over 4 per cent. At this concentration rapid breathing is apparent, and if it rises to 10 per cent, great CHANGES IN AIR AND BLOOD IN RESPIRATION. 595 distress is felt and the face becomes congested and blue. These authors conclude that expired air is injurious in itself only from the carbon dioxid it contains, and not because of any special poison. We must admit, therefore, that the existence of an organic poison in the expired air has not been conclusively demonstrated — in fact, has been made exceedingly improbable. The definitely known evil results of breathing the air of crowded, poorly ventilated rooms must be referred to other possible causes, such as the increase in temper- ature and moisture. These two conditions cause depression and malaise even when good ventilation is provided. It is possible, also, that the material given off from the skin in the perspiration, seba- ceous secretions, etc., may account sufficiently, for the odor and pos- sibly also for some of the general evil effects. If the ventilation is so poor that the carbon dioxid accumulates to the extent of 3 to 4 per cent., then this factor begins to exercise a direct effect upon the res- piratory movements and the general condition, — an effect which increases as the percentage of carbon dioxid rises. Ventilation.- — It is obvious from the foregoing statements that our knowledge is not yet sufficiently complete to enable us to say positively at what point air in a room becomes injurious to breathe, whether from products of expiration or exhalation or changes in temperature and moisture. The statement is frequently made in the books that, when the air contains as much as 1 per cent, of carbon dioxid (Smith) that has been produced by breathing, evil results, as judged by one's feelings, are sure to occur, but the ex- periments of Haldane and Smith seem to disprove this statement entirely. The practical rule in ventilation is to keep the air in chambers as nearly as possible of the composition of the atmosphere outside. Since carbon dioxid is the constituent of the air that is most easily determined the relative purity of room air is judged conveniently by quantitative estimations of this constituent. Or- dinary atmospheric air contains, on the average, 0.04 per cent, of carbon dioxid — that is, 4 parts to 10,000. The hygienists main- tain that the ventilation should be sufficiently ample to keep the carbon dioxid down to 6 parts per 10,000, thus leaving 2 parts per 10,000, 0.02 vol. per cent., as the permissible limit of vitia- tion by breathing. To determine on this basis the amount of air necessary for each person the following formula is used: d = — , in which d represents in cubic meters the delivery of fresh air per hour; e, the amount of C02 expired per hour in liters; and r the ratio of permissible vitiation of the air by C02. Assuming this latter factor, in accordance with the above statement, to be equal to 0.2, and e to be equal to 20 liters per hour (500 X 0.04 X 17 X 60), the value of d is equal to 100 cubic meters of air per hour for each person. The rapidity of renewal of air will depend naturally upon 596 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. the cubic space allotted to each individual. The smaller this space, the more ample must be the ventilation. The following figures* give an idea of the values adopted for different conditions : Amount of Ventilation Cubic Space per per Hour per Person Person in Cubic in Cubic Meters. Meters. Hospitals 60-100 30-50 Prisons 50 25 Factories 60-100 30-50 Barracks 30-50 15-25 Theaters 40-50 20-25 Halls and assembly rooms 30-60 15-30 Schools 15-20 7.5-10 Classrooms for adults 25-30 12-15 The amount of cubic space allowed is based, it will be noted, upon the supposition that the air is completely renewed by ventilation during the course of an hour. The Gases of the Blood. — The gases that are contained in the blood are oxygen, carbon dioxid, and nitrogen. These gases may be extracted completely and in a condition for quantitative analysis by means of some form of gas-pump. The principle of most of the gas-pumps used in the physiological laboratories is the same. The apparatus is arranged so that the blood to be examined is brought into a vacuum while kept at the temperature of the body. Under these conditions all of the oxygen and nitrogen and part of the car- bon dioxid are given off and may be collected by suitable means. A portion of the carbon dioxid present in the blood is in such stable combination that to remove it it may be necessary to add some dilute acid, such as phosphoric acid. This portion of the carbon dioxid is designated in this connection as the fixed carbon dioxid. The principle of the gas pump may be explained most easily by describing the simple form devised by Grehant. The essential parts of this pump are represented in Fig. 244. The mercury pump consists of two bulbs, one mov- able (M), the other fixed (F). M may be raised and lowered by the windlass (P). Above F, there is a three-way stopcock (m) by means of which the chamber F may be put into communication with the outside air by way of C, or with the bulb B, which is to contain the blood, or may be shut off com- pletely. If M is raised so as to fill F entirely, and the stopcock m is shut off, then on lowering M the mercury will flow into it, leaving a perfect vacuum in F, since the distance between F and M is greater than the barometric height. If the stopcock m is turned so as to throw F into communication with B, the chamber of this latter is brought under the influence of the vac- uum and any gases that it may contain will be distributed between B and F. If stopcock m is again turned off and M is raised the gases in F will be condensed at its upper end, and by turning the stopcock m properly these gases may be forced to the outside by way of C or may be collected, if de- sired, in a burette filled with mercury and inverted over the opening from F contained in the bottom of C. In performing an experiment the flask Bt which is to contain the blood, is connected with F, as shown in the figure, * Taken from Bergey, " The Principles of Hygiene, " 1904. CHANGES IN AIR AND BLOOD IN RESPIRATION. 597 all joints being protected from leakage by a seal of water outside, as shown at h, which represents a piece of wide rubber tubing filled with water so as to protect a joint between two pieces of glass tubing. B is next exhausted completely by raising and lowering M a number of times in the way described above until on throwing B into communication with a vacuum in F no further gas is given off. The last particles of air may be driven out from B by boil- ing a little water in it. After a complete vacuum has been established in B Fig. 244. — Gas pump for extracting the gases of blood (Grehant) : M and Ff The mercury receivers; P, the windlass for raising and lowering M; m, a three-way stopcock protected by a seal of mercury or water; C, a cup with mercury over which the receiving eudiometer is placed to collect the gases; B, the bulb in which, after a vacuum is made, the blood is introduced by the graduated syringe, S. By means of the stopcock m the vacuum in F, caused by the fall of the mercury, can be placed in communi- cation with B. After the gases have diffused over into F, M is raised, and when the stop- cock m is properly turned these gases are driven out through C into the receiving tube. The operation is repeated until no more gas is given off from B. a measured amount of blood is introduced from a graduated syringe, S, as represented in the figure. This blood must be taken directly from the vessels of the animal and be introduced into B at once. B is kept immersed in water at the temperature of the body, and the bulb M is now raised and lowered a number of times so that the gases given off from the blood are drawn over into F and then by proper manipulation of the stopcock are driven into 598 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. a burette fastened over the opening of the tube in C. To drive off all of the carbon dioxid a little dilute phosphoric acid must be added to the blood in B by means of the syringe, S. The gases thus collected into the burette are first measured and are then analyzed for the three important constituents by some of the accepted gasometric methods. The principle involved is to absorb first from the mixture all of the C02 by introducing a solution of sodium or potassium hydrate. The reading of the volume left after this absorption is completed compared with the first reading gives the volume of C02. Next, a freshly made alkaline solution of pyrogallic acid is introduced into the tube. This solution absorbs all of the oxygen, whose volume is thus easily determined. The gas that is left unabsorbed after the action of these two solutions is nitro- gen. The volumes of gases are reduced, as is the custom, to unit pressure and temperature, — that is, to zero degree centigrade and 760 mms. barometric pressure. A correction must also be made for the tension or pressure exerted by the aqueous vapor in the gases. These corrections are made by means of the following formula: yi = V(B-T) 760 X (1 + 0.003665 1) in which V1 represents the corrected volume, V the volume actually observed, B the barometric height at the time and place of the observation, T the aqueous tension at the temperature of the reading, and t the temperature in degrees centigrade. By means of such methods the gases in the blood have been de- termined. The quantities vary somewhat, of course, with the con- ditions of the animal and with the species of animal. In a quick analysis of dogs' arterial blood made by Pfluger the following figures were obtained reckoned in volumes per cent. : O, 22.6; C02, 34.3; N, 1.8. In this case each 100 c.c. of arterial blood contained 22.6 c.c. of O and 34.3 c.c. of C02 measured at 0° C. and 760 mms. Hg. An analysis of human blood (Setschenow) gave closely similar figures; O, 21.6 per cent.; C02J 40.3 per cent.; and N, 1.6 per cent. When the arterial and the venous bloods are compared it is found that the venous blood has more carbon dioxid and less oxygen. Average figures showing the difference in composition are as follows: o. co2. N. Arterial blood 20 38 1.7 Venous blood 12 45 1.7 Difference .~8 ~7 ~~0~ The actual amounts of oxygen and carbon dioxid in the venous blood vary with the nutritive activity of the tissues, and differ therefore in the various organs according to the state of activity of each organ in relation to the volume of its blood supply. This point is well illustrated by some analyses made by Hill and Na- barro* of the gases in the venous blood from the brain and the muscles, respectively. Their average results when both tissues were at rest were as follows : Oxygen. Carbon Dioxid. Venous blood from limbs (femoral) .... 6.34 per cent. 45.75 per cent. " " " brain (torcular) . . . 13.49 " " 41.65 " " *Hill and Nabarro, "Journal of Physiology," 18, 218, 1895. CHANGES IN AIR AND BLOOD IN RESPIRATION. 599 It will be seen that under similar conditions there is much less oxygen used and carbon dioxid formed in the brain than in the limbs (muscles). In the former organ the physiological oxidations must either be small compared with those of the muscles, or the brain tissues receive a relatively ample supply of blood, so that the tissue metabolism has less effect upon the blood composition. The venous blood as it comes to the lungs is a mixture of bloods from different organs, and its composition in gases will be constant only when the conditions of the body are kept uniform. Much work has been done in physiology to determine the condition in which these various gases are held in the blood. The results obtained show that they are held partly in solution and partly in chemical combination. To understand the part played by each factor and the conditions that control the exchange of gases in the lungs and tissues it is necessary to recall some facts regarding the physical and chemical properties of gases. The Pressure of Gases and the Terms Expressing these Pressures. — The air around us exists under a pressure of one atmosphere and is expressed usually in terms of the height of a column of mercury that it will support,- — namely, a column of 760 mms. Hg, which is known as the normal barometric pressure at sea-level. Air is a mixture of gases, and according to the mechani- cal theory of gases each constituent exerts a pressure corresponding to the proportion of that gas present. In atmospheric air, therefore, the oxygen, being present to the extent of 20 per cent., exerts a pressure of ■§- of an atmosphere or -g-X760= 152 mms. Hg. When we speak of one atmosphere of gas pressure, therefore, we mean a pressure equivalent to 760 mms. Hg, and in any given mixture the pressure exerted by any constituent may be expressed in percen- tages or fractions of an atmosphere or in the equivalent height of the mercury column which it will support. Absorption of Gases in Liquids. — When a gas is brought into contact with a liquid with which it does not react chemically a certain number of the moving gaseous molecules penetrate the liquid and become dissolved. Some of these dissolved molecules escape from the water from time to time, again becoming gaseous. It is evident, however, that if a liquid, water, is brought into contact with a gas under definite pressure, — that is, containing a definite number of molecules to a unit volume, — an equilibrium will be established. As many molecules will penetrate the liquid in a given time as escape from it, and the liquid will hold a definite number of the gas molecules in solution. It will be saturated for that pressure of gas. If the pressure of the gas is increased, how- ever, an equilibrium will be established at a higher level and more molecules of gas will be dissolved in the liquid. Experiments have 600 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. shown, in accordance with this mechanical conception, that the amount of a given gas dissolved by a given liquid varies, the temper- ature remaining the same, directly with the pressure, — that is, it increases and decreases proportionally with the rise and fall of the gas pressure. This is the law of Henry-Dai ton. On the other hand, the amount of gas dissolved by a liquid varies inversely with the temperature. It follows, also, from the same mechanical views that in a mixture of gases each gas is dissolved in proportion to the pressure that it exerts, and not in proportion to the pressure of the mixture. Air consists, in round numbers, of 4 parts of N and 1 part of O. Consequently, when a volume of water is exposed to the air the oxygen is dissolved a according to its " partial pressure," — that is, under a pressure of -J- of an atmosphere (152 mms. Hg). The water will contain only ^ as much oxygen as it would if exposed to a full atmosphere of oxygen — that is, to pure oxygen. And, on the other hand, if water has been saturated with oxygen at one atmosphere (760 mms.) of pressure and is then exposed to air, four-fifths of the dissolved oxygen will be given off, since the pressure of the surrounding oxygen has been diminished that much. Ab- sorption coefficient. By this term is meant the number that ex- presses the proportion of gas dissolved in a unit volume of the liquid under one atmosphere of pressure. The absorption coefficient will vary, of course, with the temperature. The gases that interest us in this connection are oxygen, nitrogen, and carbon dioxid. The absorption coefficients of these gases for the blood at the tempera- ture of the body are as follows : 0,0.0262; N, 0.0130; C02, 0.5283. That is, 1 c.c. of blood at body temperature dissolves 0.0262 of 1 c.c. of oxygen if exposed to an atmosphere of pure oxygen, and so on. The solubility of the C02 is therefore twenty times as great as that of oxygen. Accepting these figures, we may calculate how much of these three gases can be held in the arterial blood in physical solution, provided we know the pressure of the gases in the alveoli of the lungs. The composition of the alveolar air will be discussed farther on, but we may assume at present that it contains 80 per cent, of nitrogen, 15 per cent, of oxygen, and 5 per cent, of carbon dioxid. In 100 c.c. of blood, therefore, the following amounts of these gases should be held in solution: Nitrogen 100 X 0.013 X 0.80 = 1.04 c.c. Oxygen 100 X 0.0262 X 0.15 = 0.393 " Carbon dioxid 100 X 0.5283 X 0.05 = 2.64 " As will be seen from the analyses given above of the actual amounts of these gases obtained from the blood, the nitrogen alone is present in quantities corresponding to what would be expected if it is held in simple physical solution. CHANGES IN AIR AND BLOOD IN RESPIRATION. 601 The Tension or Pressure of Gases in Solution or Combi- nation.— When a gas is held in solution the equilibrium is de- stroyed if the pressure of this gas in the surrounding medium or atmosphere is changed. If this pressure is increased the liquid takes up more of the gas, and an equilibrium is established at a higher level. If the pressure is decreased the liquid gives off some -of the gas. That pressure of the gas in the surrounding atmosphere at which equilibrium is established measures the tension of the gas in the liquid at that time. Thus, when a bowl of water is exposed to the air the tension of the oxygen in solution is 152 mms. Hg; that of the nitrogen is 608 mms. Hg. If the same water is exposed to pure oxygen the tension of the oxygen in solution is equal to 760 mms. Hg, while that of the nitrogen sinks to zero if the gas that is given off from the water is removed. With compounds such as oxyhemoglobin the tension under which the oxygen is held is measured by the pressure of the gas in the sur- rounding atmosphere at which the compound neither takes up nor gives off oxygen. If, therefore, it is necessary to determine the tension of any gas held in solution or in dissociable combination it is sufficient to determine the percentage of that gas in the surrounding atmosphere and thus ascertain the partial pressure that it exerts. If the atmosphere contains 5 per cent, of the gas the partial pressure is equal to 38 mms. Hg (760 X 0.05), and this figure expresses the tension under which the gas is held in solution or combination. As regards the tension of the gases in arterial and venous blood, this procedure is, of course, not possible, since the blood is sur- rounded, not by an atmosphere whose composition can be analyzed, but by the liquids of the body, the lymph and cell juices. To determine the tension of the gases in the blood it is necessary to remove the blood from the vessels and bring it into contact with an atmosphere containing a known quantity of O, C02, or N, according to the gas to be measured. By trial an atmosphere can be obtained in which this gas is contained in amounts such that there is no marked increase or decrease in quantity after standing in diffusion relations with the blood. The percentage of the gas in the atmos- phere chosen will measure the tension of that gas in the blood. An instrument which has been much used for such determinations is represented diagrammatically in Fig. 245. It is known as an aero- tonometer (Pfluger). It consists of a tube (A) which can be con- nected through b directly with the blood-vessels. This tube A is surrounded by a jacket (C) containing warm water, so that the blood may be kept at the body temperature during the experiment. A is first completely filled with mercury from the bulb M to drive out the air. An atmosphere of known composition A by dropping the bulb. Blood is allowed 602 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. the stopcock b and to trickle down the sides of the tube. Diffusion relations are set up between the blood and the known atmosphere, and after equilibrium has been established the gas is driven out through a into a convenient receiver and analyzed. If two aero- tonometers are used, one containing the gas at somewhat higher pressure than that expected, and the other at a somewhat lower pressure, an average result is ob- tained which expresses with suffi- cient accuracy the pressure of the given gas in the blood. It is important not to confuse the tension at which a gas is held in a liquid with the volume of the gas. Thus, blood exposed to the air contains its oxygen under a tension of 152 mms. Hg, but the amount of oxygen is equal to 20 volumes per cent. Water exposed to the air contains its oxygen un- der the same tension, but the amount of gas in solution is less than 1 volume per cent. Tensions of gases in liquids are expressed either in percentages of an atmos- phere or in millimeters of mercury. Thus, the tension of oxygen in ar- terial blood is found to be equal to about 10 per cent, of an atmos- phere or 76 mms. Hg. (760 X 0.10). The Condition and Signifi- cance of the Nitrogen. — We may accept the view that the nitrogen of the blood is held in physical solution. The amount present corresponds with this view, and, moreover, it is found that the quantity varies directly with the pressure in accordance with the law of Henry and Dal ton. If an animal is permitted to breathe an atmosphere of oxygen and hydrogen the nitrogen disappears from the blood, and the nitrogen contents of the arterial and venous bloods exhibit no constant difference in quantity. It seems certain, therefore, that the nitro- gen plays no direct role in the physiological processes. It is ab- sorbed by the blood in proportion to its partial pressure in the Fig. 245. — Diagram to show the principle of the aerotonometer : A, The tube containing a known mixture of gases, O, COj, N; C, the outside jacket for maintaining a constant body tem- perature. When stopcock b is open the blood trickles down the sides of A and enters into diffusion relations with the contained gases. After equilibrium is reached the stopcock b is closed and a is opened. By means of the mer- cury bulb the gases can then be forced out of A into a suitable receiver for analysis. CHANGES IN AIR AND BLOOD IN RESPIRATION. 603 alveoli of the lungs and circulates in the blood in small amounts without exerting any immediate influence upon the tissues. Condition of Oxygen in the Blood. — That the oxygen is not held in the blood merely in solution is indicated, in the first place, by the large quantity present and, in the second place, by the fact that this quantity does not vary directly with the pressure in the surrounding medium. It is definitely known that by far the largest portion of the oxygen is held in chemical combination with the hemoglobin of the red corpuscles, while a much smaller portion, varying with the pressure, is held in solution in the plasma. The compound oxyhemoglobin possesses the important property that when the pressure of oxygen in the surrounding medium falls suffi- ciently it begins to dissociate and free oxygen is given off. The proc- I a i » 2 n.C«nl>*l Em! Vagus- Weak, Fig. 248. — To show the inhibition of the resoiratory movements in a rabbit due to stimulation of the central end of the vagus. The respiratory movements in this case, before and after stimulation, were forced, owing to the fact that both vagi were cut. be changed together with an increased or decreased amplitude, the inspirations and expirations may each be increased, or one phase 614 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. may be affected more markedly than the other. In general, how- ever, experimental stimulation of a sensory nerve trunk which con- tains cutaneous fibers gives one of two effects : either a stimulating action, manifested by quicker, stronger inspirations and active ex- pirations, or an inhibitory effect, in which the respirations cease altogether or become slower and more feeble (Figs. 247 and 248). If in this, as in other similar cases, we assume that the two oppo- site effects are produced by different nerve fibers we may speak of sensory fibers which have a stimulating or augmenting effect, and of those that have an inhibiting influence on the center, or following the terminology used in the case of the vasomotor center, we may speak of respiratory pressor and respiratory depressor fibers. It is quite probable that these fibers have other functions, — that is, they are not distributed exclusively to the respiratory center. A cuta- neous fiber, which through its central chain of neurons eventually ends in the cortex cerebri and gives us a sensation of pain, may by collateral connections affect also the medullary center and pro- duce effects upon the heart, blood-vessels, and respirations. The Special Relations of the Afferent Fibers of the Vagus to the Center. — Although the sensory nerves in general exert a reflex effect upon the respiratory center, experimental work has shown that the sensory fibers distributed along the respiratory passages from the anterior nares to the alveoli have a specially important relation to this center. This fact is most clearly shown in the case of the sensory fibers of the vagus, which are distributed to the lungs themselves. If the two vagi are cut in the neck the respiratory movements are at once altered in character; they show a much slower rhythm and greater amplitude (Fig. 249). The inspirations especially are deeper and longer, with something of a pause at the end. When only one vagus is cut an intermediate effect may be obtained, the respiratory movements may be slowed somewhat and slightly deepened ; but the striking effect is obtained only after section of both nerves. This result is not a temporary one due to the stimulation of cutting, but is permanent, and there- fore leads to the conclusion that some influence has been cut off which normally keeps the respiratory movements at a more rapid rate. Experiment has shown that this influence consists in the tonic action of sensory fibers contained in the vagus and distributed to the lungs. It is the constant effect of these fibers on the respira- tory center which maintains the normal rhythm; when they are severed the center drops into a slower, unregulated rhythm. Ex- periment has shown, also, that when the central stump of the divided vagus is stimulated artificially the respiratory center is affected, as indicated by the respiratory movements, in a variety of ways which depend upon the strength of the stimulus and the INNERVATION OF THE RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 615 condition of the center. The two results which are most constantly obtained and which may therefore be especially emphasized are as follows: first, with weak stimuli the inspiratory movements are in- hibited partially or completely, giving either smaller movements or, in a condition of narcosis, complete cessation of respirations, with Fig. 249. — To show the effect of section of the vagi on the respiratory movements (rabbit). The right vagus was cut at x and caused a slight augmentation and slowing of the movements. The left vagus was cut at xx and caused first a short inhibition (due to mechanical stimulation) which was then followed by the typical slow and deep respi- rations seen under these conditions. — (Dawson.) the thorax in the stage of passive expiration (Fig. 248), or, second, the rate of the inspiratory movements ma}^ be increased and this may end finally in an inspiratory standstill, — that is, the respiratory movements cease with the chest in an inspiratory position (Fig. 250), Fig. 250. — To illustrate the inspiratory effect from stimulation of the central end of the vagus. The downstroke represents inspiration; the upstroke, expiration. During the period of stimulation the respirations are increased in frequency and the chest remains in a condition of inspiration. — (Lewandowsky.) the inspiratory muscles being in a condition of tetanic contraction. When both the inspiratory and expiratory muscles are considered, the variety of effects that may be obtained from stimulation of the afferent fibers of the vagus is perplexing, especially with strong 616 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. stimuli, and has led to much difference of opinion among investi- gators.* The two main effects described above and obtained with stimuli not too strong are usually interpreted to mean that the vagus contains two kinds of sensory fibers which are distributed to the lungs and act normally on the respiratory center. These are: (I) The inspiratory fibers, whose effect is to increase the rate of inspiratory discharge from the respiratory center; therefore to quicken the rate. (II) The expiratory (or inspiratory inhibiting) fibers, whose effect is to inhibit the inspiratory discharges, partially or completely. Some authors find it simpler to assume only one kind of sensory fiber and to explain the different results by a dif- ference in the nature of the stimulus or in the condition of the center; but it seems advisable at present, in accordance with the doctrine of specific nerve energies, to hold to the view of two varieties. Influence of the Inspiratory and the Inhibitory Fibers of the Vagus on the Normal Respirations. — It is assumed that these two sets of fibers are in constant activity and keep the re- spiratory rate more rapid than it would be otherwise. Hence the slowing and deepening of the respirations when the vagi are cut. The way in which these sensory fibers are stimulated normally was referred by Hering and Breuer to the alternate expansion and collapse of the lungs. Each inspiration stimulates the inhibitory fibers in consequence of the expansion of the lungs, and thus cuts short the inspiration, prematurely, as it were. So at each expira- tion the collapse of the lungs stimulates the inspiratory fibers and brings on an inspiration sooner than would otherwise occur. In this way the respiratory rate is kept automatically at an accel- erated rhythm. A stimulation of the sensory fibers as a result of expansion of the lungs is easy to comprehend and, indeed, it has been demonstrated by showing that with each expansion an action current may be obtained in the vagus by means of the galvanometer or capillary electrometer. But that the normal collapse of the lungs also acts as a mechanical stimulus to a different set of nerve endings is not such a probable hypothesis, and most physiologists believe that it is not necessary to adopt it, — at least for normal respirations. Head and also Schenck have shown that with a certain extreme extent of collapse evidence may be obtained of a stimulation of the inspiratory fibers. We may assume, with Gad, that the normal rate of respirations is maintained by the action of the inhibitory fibers alone. Each inspiration is cut short by the mechanical stimulation of these fibers, but on the collapse of * For discussion and literature see Meltzer, " Archiv f . Physiologie, " 1892, p. 340; also "New York Medical Journal, " January 18, 1890. Le- wandowsky, " Archiv f. Physiologie," 1896, pp. 195 and 483. INNERVATION OF THE RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 617 the lungs the new inspiration is due to a normal discharge from the inspiratory center. Loewy* has shown by an ingenious experiment that the expansion of the lungs is the factor that actually stimulates the sensory fibers and quickens the respiratory rate, as follows : An animal was made to breathe pure oxygen for a while to displace the nitrogen in the alveoli. The chest on one side — say, the right side — was then opened with the result that the lung collapsed, and, owing to the rapid absorption of the oxygen, soon became practically solid. The respirations (rabbit) showed their normal rate — 66. The vagus nerve on the left side was then cut and immediately the respirations took on the character usually shown when both vagi are severed, — respirations = 34. Next the collapsed right lung was expanded by artificial respiration, with the result that the respiratory rate at once returned to normal. Respiratory Reflexes from the Larynx, Pharynx, and Nose. — The mucous membrane of the larynx receives its sensory fibers from the superior laryngeal nerve. When this nerve is stimulated artificially the respirations are always inhibited ; the chest comes to rest in the position of passive expiration. The same effect may be obtained from the sensory fibers of the glossopharyngeal supplying the pharynx, and indeed a temporary inhibition of respirations occurs through this nerve during every act of swallowing. The sensory fibers of the nasal mucous membrane (trigeminal) cause a similar reflex inhibition when stimulated by injurious or so called irrespirable gases, such as HC1, CI, NH3, S02, etc. We may regard this inhibitory influence exerted by the sensory fibers distributed along the air passages as a protective reflex which guards the lungs automatically from injurious gases. This protective action is made more evident by the fact that, together with the cessation of respirations, the glottis is reflexly closed by contraction of the ad- ductor muscles and, if the stimulation is strong, even the bronchial musculature may be contracted, so that in every way the passage to the alveoli is made more difficult. The reflex is, of course, more or less temporary, but it possesses the great advantage of being automatic, and may enable the animal or individual to escape unharmed from a dangerous locality before the increasing irritabil- ity of the respiratory center breaks through the inhibition. In special cases the inhibition may last for an unusually long time. »Thus, Fredericq states that in aquatic birds water allowed to flow over the beak so as to penetrate slightly into the nostrils brings about an inhibition of respirations for many minutes. There would seem in this case to be a special adaptation of the reflex to the needs of diving. We know also that irritating gases or foreign bodies of any sort that enter the larynx may lead to a coughing reflex, — that is, to a series of expiratory blasts which have a pur- Iposeful end in the expulsion of the stimulating object. In this case * "Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 42, 273. ■ 618 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. there is not simply an inhibition of the inspiratory movements, but a reflex excitation of a peculiar type of expiratory movements. The Voluntary Control of the Respiratory Movements. — We can control the respiratory movements within wide limits, make forced or feeble inspirations or expirations, accelerate the rhythm, or completely inhibit the respirations in any phase. If, however, the "breath is held," — that is, if the respiratory movements are inhibited and the glottis is closed, the increasing irritability of the respiratory center eventually breaks through the voluntary inhibition. How far this voluntary control is based upon direct connections between the cerebrum and the respiratory center and how far it depends upon voluntary paths to the separate spinal nuclei of the muscles involved cannot be discussed profitably. The Nature of the Respiratory Center. — The respiratory center located in the medulla oblongata might with more propriety be designated as the inspiratory center. Our normal respirations throughout life consist of an active inspiration and a passive expiration. It is the co-ordinated activity of the inspiratory muscles that is characteristic of the respiratory movements. The expiratory muscles come into action only occasionally and under special conditions. It is, in reality, incorrect to speak of the normal respirations as consisting of alternate inspiratory and expiratory movements; as a matter of fact, they consist of rhythmical in- spiratory movements alone. So also when we describe the respira- tory center as essentially automatic we refer only to the action on the inspiratory muscles, since a series of active inspiratory move- ments is the essential feature of respiration. Under certain con- ditions, however, we do have rhythmical expiratory movements, active expirations. Such movements may occur independently of the respirations proper, as in coughing and laughing, or in the straining movements of defecation, micturition, and parturition; or they may occur as an integral part of the respirations, as in the forced movements of dyspnea. Under the conditions of partial suffocation, for instance, as the blood becomes more and more venous the respirations increase in force and active expirations appear. It becomes a question, therefore, as to the existence of what might be called an expiratory center, a group of nerve cells controlling the co-ordinated activity of the expiratory muscles. The mere fact that in dyspnea we have a rhythmical and co-ordi- nated activity of these muscles seems to imply the existence of such a center, but there is no definite experimental knowledge as to its location. Assuming that there is such a center, it may be believed that it exists in the medulla, since after section below the medulla there is no evidence of the occurrence of rhythmical expiratory movements even in extreme conditions of venosity of the blood. INNERVATION OF THE RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 619 The expiratory center may or may not be located in the same region as the inspiratory center, but the following general char- acteristics may be assigned to it : In the first place, it is not auto- matic; at least not under normal conditions. In the second place, its activity must be dependent in some way upon that of the in- spiratory center. Even our most violent respiratory movements show an orderly sequence of inspiration and expiration, — and we may believe that the action of the expiratory center is conditioned by the previous discharge of the inspiratory center, just as in the heart the beat of the ventricle depends upon the previous systole of the auricle. That an active expiration is not caused reflexly by the mechanical expansion of the lungs seems to be demonstrated by the fact that the most forcible voluntary inspiration is followed by a passive, not an active expiration. Until our knowledge is extended by further experimental work we may consider the ex- piratory center as a group of cells connected by definite paths with the expiratory muscles and capable of being stimulated in one of at least four general ways: (1) In special reflexes, such as coughing. (2) By voluntary control from the cerebrum, as in straining. (3) By stimulation through afferent fibers from the skin, especially the pain fibers. (4) By the action of an increased venosity of the blood. Under the latter two conditions it is possible that the irritability of the center is so increased that it becomes responsive to the in- fluence of the inspiratory center. The relations of the inspiratory and expiratory centers under the various conditions of artificial stimulation are very complex, and although it is possible to rep- resent these relations more or less completely by a schema of some sort it does not seem advisable at present to seriously consider such hypotheses. The Accessory Respiratory Centers of the Midbrain. — Several observers have called attention to the existence of a possible accessory respiratory center in the midbrain at the level of the posterior colliculus. Martin and Booker found that stimulations in this region caused a marked increase in the rate of inspiratory movements and finally a standstill in inspiration, — that is, a complete tetanic contraction of the inspiratory muscles lasting during the stimulation.* Lewandowskyf has shown that section of the brain stem at or below the inferior colliculi causes an alteration in the respiratory rhythm similar to that following section of both vagi. After cutting through the inferior colliculi further sections more posteriorly do not add to the effect. He considers that there is an automatic inhibitory center in the midbrain which influences continually the automatic activity of the medullary center. The Nature of the Automatic Stimulus to the Respiratory Center. — We have accepted the view that the respiratory (inspira- tory) center is essentially automatic, although very sensitive to * Martin and Booker, ''Journal of Physiology," 1, 370, 1878. t "Archiv f . Physiologie, " 1896, 489. 620 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. reflex stimulation. The further question arises as to the nature of the automatic stimulus. Inasmuch as the activity of the center controls the gaseous exchanges of the blood, it was natural perhaps for physiologists to look to the gases of the blood for the origin of the internal stimulus. Experiments show beyond question that the condition of the gases in the blood has a direct and marked influence upon the activity of the center. If for any reason the blood supplying the center becomes more venous, the respirations are increased in force or rate or both, and indeed the activity of the center is in a general way increased in proportion to the venosity of the blood. On the other hand, if the blood supplying the center is more arterialized than normal, by active ventilation of the lungs, the center acts more feebly or may fail to act altogether, giving the condition known as apnea. These facts may be accepted as com- pletely demonstrated, but they do not go far enough. When we speak of the arterial blood being more venous than normal we mean that it contains less oxygen, and more carbon dioxid than normal arterial blood. Which of these conditions serves to stimulate the center, and which may be regarded as the constant stimulus through- out life? The three possible views have been defended : (1) That the normal stimulus is a lack of sufficient oxygen (Rosenthal). When sufficient O is supplied the center ceases to act, becomes apneic. (2) That the normal stimulus is the presence of an excess of C02 (Traube). When this excretion is quickly removed the cen- ter ceases to act, — becomes apneic. (3) It is possible that the two factors may co-operate. The blood that flows through the center may stimulate the cells by virtue of the fact that it does not remove the C02 fast enough and does not supply sufficient oxygen. It is difficult, indeed impossible, to arrive at any certain conclusion upon this point. Much evidence has been collected to show that the action of the respiratory center is increased when the tension of the C02 in the blood is raised without altering that of the oxygen and that a similar result is obtained if the tension of oxygen is greatly diminished without any change in that of the carbon dioxid, so that it must be admitted that a change in either factor, if suffi- ciently great, acts as a stimulus. Experiments, however, have indicated that the accumulation of the C02 is the more efficient stimulus of the two.* Zuntz reports the following interesting experiments, in which the extent of the respiratory movements was measured by the amount of air breathed in a minute. In one series the amount of oxygen in the air breathed was reduced. This change did not affect the quantity of carbon dioxid in the blood. The following results were obtained: *See Zuntz, "Archiv f. Physiologie," 1897, 379. See also Friedlander and Herter, "Zeit. f. physiol. Chemie," 2, 99, and 3, 19. INNERVATION OF THE RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 621 Normal air volume breathed per minute = 7,325 to 9,000 c.c. Air with 10 to 11.5 per cent, oxygen " " " " = 8,166 to 9,428 " Air with 8 to 10 per cent, oxygen " " " " = 9,093 to 12,810 " A reduction of one-half of the oxygen in the air breathed had little effect upon the respirations. From our present standpoint, how- ever, the important thing is not the amount of oxygen in the air, but the amount in the blood. Paul Bert's experiments* upon living animals indicate that when the oxygen of the air is reduced by a half the amount of oxygen in the blood is diminished by about one-third. Assuming this to be correct, it is evident that a very considerable reduction may be made in the oxygen of the blood without noticeably affecting the respirations. A similar conclusion may be drawn from Haldane's experiments f with carbon monoxid. He found upon breathing mixtures of this gas that no distinct effects were observable until the blood was about one-third saturated with the gas — that is, had lost one-third of its oxygen. Zuntz's ex- periments, in which the C02 in the air breathed was increased, while the oxygen remained normal, gave quite different results, as follows : Normal air volume breathed per minute, 7,433 c.c. Air of 20.2 per cent. O, 0.95 per cent. C02 " " " " 9.060 " Air of 18.06 per cent. O, 2.97 per cent. C02 " " " " 11,326 " Air of 18.42 per cent. O, 11.5 per cent. C02 " " " " 32,464 " These and similar results show that small differences in the amount of the carbon dioxid in the blood have a distinct effect upon the activity of the respirator}^ center. Under normal con- ditions the respiratory center receives blood containing 19 to 20 volumes per cent, of oxygen, while the venous blood flowing away from the center still holds 10 to 12 per cent. Considering the small effect of lowering this oxygen supply by one-third, it is difficult to believe that normally the amount of oxygen is so deficient for the normal metabolism as to set up a constant stimulus. The facts seem to favor rather the view that it is the constant presence of the C02 which directly or indirectly occasions the normal discharges from the respiratory center. It is, of course, possible that other constituents of the blood may play an important part in providing accessory conditions of rhythmical activity, as in the case of the heart beat. The Cause of the First Respiratory Movement. — The mam- malian fetus under normal conditions makes no respiratory move- * Bert, "La pression barometrique, " 1878, 691. f Haldane, " Journal of Physiology, " 18, 442, 1895. 622 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. ments while in utero. After birth and the interruption of the pla- cental circulation the first breath is taken. The cause of this sudden awakening to activity on the part of the respiratory center must be closely connected, if not identical with, the cause of the automatic activity of the center throughout life. Two or perhaps three views have been held regarding its immediate cause: (1) That it is due to the increased venosity of the blood brought about by the interruption of the placental circulation; (2) that it is due to stimulation of the skin by handling, drying, etc.; (3) that it is due to a combination of these causes. Preyer has shown that stimula- tion of the skin of the fetus while in utero and with the placental circulation intact sufficies to cause respiratory movements. Cohn- stein and Zuntz* have shown that interruption of the placental circulation while the fetus is kept bathed in the amniotic liquid also brings about respirations. Since both of these events occur normally at birth, we may believe that each aids in causing the first respira- tion, and indeed it may be necessary at times deliberately to in- crease the stimulation of the skin in order to bring on respiratory movements. If the two causes, stimulation through the nerves and stimulation through the blood, normally co-operate, it may, how- ever, be said that the essential cause, according to the theory adopted in the preceding paragraphs, lies in the greater venosity of the blood following interruption of the placental circulation. During the intra-uterine period it is evident that the fetal blood is aerated by exchange with the maternal blood sufficiently well not to act as a stimulus to the fetal respiratory center. The fetus is, physiologically speaking, in a condition of apnea. Since the maternal blood acts upon the respiratory center of the mother, while the fetal blood which exchanges gases with it does not act on its own respiratory center, it follows that the fetal respiratory center possesses a lower degree of irritability than that of the mother. Dyspnea, Hyperpnea, Apnea. — By the term dyspnea in its widest sense we mean any noticeable increase in the force or rate of the respiratory movements. As said above, such a condition may be caused either by stimulation of sensory nerves, particularly the pain nerves, or by an increased venosity of the blood, — that is, by an increase in the C02 or by a marked decrease in the oxygen. Changes of other kinds in the composition of the blood, some of which are considered in the next chapter, may also stimulate the respiratory center and cause dyspnea. The dyspneic movements naturally show many degrees of intensity corresponding with the strength of the stimulus, and sometimes the initial stages are desig- nated as hyperpnea, while the term dyspnea is reserved for the more * Cohnstein and Zuntz, " Arch. f. die gesammte Physiol," 42, 342, 1888. INNERVATION OF THE RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 623 labored breathing in which the expirations are active and forced. When dyspnea is produced by withholding air (suffocation) the respiratory movements become more and more violent until they take on a convulsive character. This stage is succeeded by one of apparent calm, indicative of exhaustion of the centers. Deep, long-drawn inspirations follow at intervals and finally cease. The animal lies quietly, with feeble heart beat and dilated pupils, in a condition designated as asphyxia or complete asphyxia. The term apnea means literally a condition of no breathing, and since this condition may occur from several causes some confusion in nomenclature has resulted. In medical literature the term is some- times employed as a synonym for asphyxia or suffocation. In physiological literature it is restricted to a very interesting con- Fig. 251. — To show the recovery from apnea. The animal (rabbit) had been venti- lated with a bellows and thrown into a condition of apnea shown at the beginning of the record. The respirations returned first as feeble movements which gradually in- creased to the normal. — (Dawson.) dition which is of great importance with reference to the theories of respiration. This condition is one of cessation of breathing movements due to lack of stimulation of the respiratory center. It is brought about by rapid and prolonged ventilation of the lungs. If, for instance, in a rabbit or other animal, a tracheal cannula is inserted and connected with a bellows or respiration apparatus, the lungs may be inflated artificially at a rapid rate for any given period of time. If such an experiment is per- formed it will be found that when the blasts are stopped the animal makes no breathing movements at all, sometimes for a considerable interval. When the respirations start again they begin with feeble movements, which gradually increase to the normal amplitude (Fig. 251). One may produce a similar condition upon 624 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. himself, approximately at least, by a series of rapid, forced inspira- tions. The question of importance is: Why does the respiratory center cease to act? Rosenthal explained the phenomenon in terms of his theory that the normal stimulus to the center results from a lack of oxygen. With vigorous artificial respiration he imagined that the blood takes up more oxygen and thus fails to act upon the center. The apnea is due to overoxygenation of the blood, and indeed this is the definition he gave to the word.* The numerous researches made upon this condition seem to show very clearly that in the method used to produce it two factors co-operate, and that it is necessary, in reality, to distinguish two different kinds of apnea, the apncea vera or chemical apnea, and the apncea vagi or inhibitory apnea. When the lungs are vigorously inflated by artificial blasts the alveoli are better ventilated; and consequently the blood takes up somewhat more of oxygen and gives off more carbon dioxid. It reaches the center in what may be called a more arterialized or less venous condition. At the same time the repeated expansions of the lungs cause repeated stimula- tions of the inhibitory fibers in the vagus, and this tends to bring the center to rest by inhibition. Either of these factors alone may cause a condition of apnea and in the method by which the phenom- enon is usually produced the two co-operate, as may be inferred from the following facts: If the vagi are cut it is much more difficult to produce apnea by artificial respirations. If in an animal with vagi intact the artificial respirations are made with hydrogen in- stead of air an apneic pause may be obtained, but this is no longer possible if the vagi are cut.f These two facts indicate the impor- tance of the inhibitory factor. That chemical apnea in Rosen- thal's sense may exist is shown by the fact that after section of both vagi apnea may still be produced by artificial respiration, and indeed several observers^ find that after section of both vagi and of the medulla above the center the animal may still be made apneic. In such cases it is difficult to see any other cause for the apnea than a change in the gases of the blood. Rosenthal assumed that this latter condition is due to an overoxygenation of the blood, but since the vigorous respirations lower the contents of the blood in C02 it is possible, as insisted upon by Traube, that this factor may be the more important. At present there are no facts which will enable us to decide definitively between these views; but, since, in the preceding paragraphs, some evidence has been given to show that the normal stimulus to the center is due to the * See Rosenthal, in vol. iv, p. 264, of Hermann's "Handbuch der Physi- ologic " t See Head, "Journal of Physiology," 10, 1, and 279, 1889. j Loewy, " Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 42, 245, 1888; and Langendorff, "Archiv f. Physiologie," 1888, p. 286. INNERVATION OF THE RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 625 presence of C02, it follows logically that the more complete removal of this gas by ventilation of the lungs should be considered as the chief cause of true apnea. Experimentally this view is well borne out by an old observation of Berns, according to which a condition of apnea in a rabbit may be cut short instantly at any moment by a blast of C02 sent into the lungs, a blast of air having no such affect. This observation is further supported by recent experiments by Mosso* upon men, in which he shows that apnea can not be produced by inflation with carbon dioxid. This author designates the condition of diminished C02 in the blood as acapnia. According to this terminology, true apnea is due to a condition of acapnia. In the intact animal, therefore, we may say that apnea is due to two causes: first, the removal of C02 from the blood by better ventilation, whereby the center is stimulated less strongly or not at all; and, second, the rhythmical inhibition of the center through the vagus fibers ending in the lungs. The two causes work together, and, as it were, aid each other, for, the less the irritability of the center, the more easily it is inhibited, and, the more it is inhibited, the less the internal stimulus affects it. Innervation of the Bronchial Musculature. — Numerous investigators, using different methods, have demonstrated that the bronchial musculature is supplied through the vagus with motor and inhibitory fibers, bronchoconstrictor and bronchodilator fibers, as they are usually called. t Stimulation of the constrictors causes a narrowing of the bronchi, and therefore increases the resistance to the inflow and outflow of air. Some observers state that these fibers are normally in a condition of tonic activity (Roy and Brown), but others find little evidence for this belief. An artificial tonus — that is, a condition of maintained activity of the constrictor fibers — may be set up by the action of a number of drugs, such as muscarin, pilocarpin, and physostigmin, which in this case, as in so many other instances of autonomic fibers, are supposed to stimulate the endings of the fibers in the lungs. Their effect is removed by the action of atropin. These fibers are stimulated also during the ex- citatory stages of asphyxia. Reflex stimulation of the constrictors is obtained most readily (Dixon and Brodie) by irritation of the nasal mucous membrane, and it seems probable that in bronchial or spasmodic asthma these fibers are also stimulated reflexly. The normal conditions under which the constrictors and dilators are brought into play can scarcely be stated. Irritating vapors or even C02 lead to a bronchoconstriction and this reflex, as stated on * Mosso, "Archives italiennes de biologie," 40, 1, 1903. t For a recent paper with references to literature see Dixon and Brodie, "Journal of Physiology," 29, 97, 1903. 40 626 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. p. 617, may be regarded as protective. When a constriction of the bronchial musculature exists it may be abolished by the paralyzing action of atropin, or temporarily by injections of extracts of lobelia or by the anesthetic effect of inhalations of chloroform or ether. Nicotin also causes a dilatation. CHAPTER XXXVIII. THE INFLUENCE OF VARIOUS CONDITIONS UPON THE RESPIRATIONS. The Effect of Muscular Work upon the Respiratory Move- ments.— It is a matter of common experience that muscular, ex- ercise increases the rate and amplitude of the respiratory move- ments. Roughly speaking, the increase is proportional to the amount of muscular work, and the relationship is evidently a bene- ficial adaptation. The greater the amount of work done, the larger will be the amount of C02 produced and the greater will be the need of oxygen. The adaptation was formerly explained in what seemed to be an entirely satisfactory way by assuming that the increased consumption of O and the greater production of C02 in the muscles resulted in rendering the blood more venous, and consequently the respiratory center was stimulated more strongly, and indeed proportionally to the muscular effort. Geppert and Zuntz,* however, have shown by gas analyses that whatever may be the condition of the venous blood during muscular exercise the arterial blood sent out from the left heart shows no constant change in the quantity or tension of the contained gases. They proved, also, that the effect on the center is not simply a reflex from the nerves in the muscles, since when the hind limbs were made to contract by stimulation the respiratory center was affected in the usual way although all the nerve connections were destroyed. They conclude, therefore, that the respiratory effect of muscular work must be due to certain substances produced in the muscle and given off to the blood. Other experiments (Lehmann) make it probable that these substances are the acid products, lactic acid and acid phosphates, known to be formed in muscle during contraction. Dilute acids injected directly into the veins produce a similar result. The adaptation is a most interesting one, since the products that decrease the irritability of the muscle itself seem to cause an increase in excitability of the group of nerve cells constituting the respiratory center. The Effect of Variations in the Composition of the Air Breathed. — Variations in the amount of nitrogen in the inspired * Geppert and Zuntz, " Archiv f . die gesammte Physiologie, " 42, 189, 1888. 627 628 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. air have no distinct physiological effect. The important elements to consider are the oxygen and the carbon dioxid. Increased Percentages of Oxygen. — The normal pressure of oxygen in the air is 20 per cent, or 152 mms. We may increase this pres- sure either by changing the volume per cent, of the gas or by raising the barometric pressure by compression. The somewhat natural supposition that breathing pure oxygen — that is, oxygen at a pres- sure of 760 mm. — should have a beneficial effect on the oxidations of the body has found no support in physiological experiments. Atmospheric air supplies us with an excess of oxygen over the needs of the body; a still further increase of this excess has no positive advantage. Paul Bert, in his interesting work on barometric pres- sures,* has called attention to the fact that at a certain pressure oxygen is not only not beneficial, but, on the contrary, is markedly toxic. From experiments made upon a great variety of animals and plants he concluded that all living things are killed when the oxygen pressure is sufficiently high, — say, 300 to 400 per cent. Warm-blooded animals die with convulsions when submitted to 3 atmospheres of pure oxygen or 15 atmospheres of air. At these high pressures the blood contains about 30 volumes of oxygen to each 100 c.c. of blood instead of the usual 20 volumes. The ad- ditional 10 volumes are contained in solution. Fish also are killed when the oxygen pressure is increased to such a point that the water contains 10 volumes of dissolved oxygen to each 100 c.c. In more recent experiments by Smith, f made upon mice, it was found that oxygen at pressures of 100 per cent, to 130 per cent, proves fatal in a few days, the animals showing inflammatory changes in the lungs. Oxygen at 180 per cent, kills mice and birds within twenty- four hours. Pressures of two atmospheres of air (40 per cent. O) have no injurious effect. No adequate chemical explanation can be offered at present for this toxic action of oxygen at high tensions. The matter is one of practical importance in connection with caisson and submarine work and the therapeutical use of oxygen. Decreased Percentages of Oxygen. — Numerous observers (Bert, Zuntz, et al.) have shown that a fall in oxygen pressure has no perceptibly injurious result until it reaches about 10 per cent. At or somewhat below this pressure the hemoglobin is unable to take up its full amount of oxygen, and the body consequently suffers from a real deficiency in its oxygen supply, a condition designated as anoxemia. According to Bert's experimental results, death with convulsions quickly follows a fall of atmospheric pressure to 250 mms. (oxygen pressure, 50 mms. or 6 to 7 per cent.). Animals supplied with an atmosphere containing a deficient amount of * " La pression barometrique, " p. 764, Paris, 1878. t "Journal of Physiology," 24, 19, 1899. INFLUENCE OF VARIOUS CONDITIONS ON RESPIRATION. 629 oxygen show dyspneic respirations, which increase in violence and finally become convulsive. The ordinary symptoms described for death from asphyxia are due, therefore, to the anoxemia, — that is, lack of oxygen, — not to the accumulation of C02. Increased Percentages of Carbon Dioxid. — It was pointed out clearly by the researches of Friedlander and Herter* that death from increased percentages of C02 is accompanied by symptoms quite different from those due to lack of oxygen. As the C02 is increased a noticeable hyperpnea may be observed (Zuntz) at a concentration of about 3 per cent. When the concentration of C02 reaches 8 per cent, to 10 or 15 per cent, there is distinct dyspnea; but beyond this point further concentration, instead of augmenting the respirations, decreases them, and the animal dies, at concen- trations of 40 to 50 per cent., without convulsions, but with the appearance, rather, of a fatal narcosis. High and Low Barometric Pressures, Mountain Sickness, Caisson Disease, etc. — High barometric pressures are used in submarine work, diving, caisson work, etc. As stated above, it follows from the work of Bert and Smith that when the pressure reaches 5 to 6 atmospheres long continuance in it may be followed by injurious or fatal results due to the toxic action of the oxygen. If the pressure is increased to 15 atmospheres the toxic influence of the oxygen brings on death with convulsions. Practically, however, such pressures are not encountered in submarine work. A caisson is a wooden or steel chamber arranged so that it may be sunk under water. The water is driven out by air under pres- sure. Since the pressure increases 1 atmosphere for each 10 meters (33 feet), it will be seen that very high pressures of air are not usually required. Caisson workers are at times attacked by serious or even fatal symptoms, not while in the compressed air, but during or after the " decompression " that is necessary in the return to normal conditions. The symptoms consist of pains in the muscles and joints, paralysis, dyspnea, congestion. Those who have investigated the subjectf state that the injurious results are due to a too rapid decompression. When this occurs the gases in the blood, particularly the nitrogen, are suddenly liberated as bubbles, which block the capillaries and thus produce anemia in different organs. If the decompression is effected gradually no evil results follow. The effect of low barometric pressures is chiefly of interest in connection with residence in high altitudes, balloon ascensions, * Friedlander and Herter, " Zeitschrift f . physiol. Chemie, " 2, 99, 1878, and 3. 19, 1879. t See Bert, loc. cit., p. 939; also Hill and MacLeod, " Journal of Physi- ology," 29, 382, and "Journal of Hygiene," 3, 407. 630 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. etc. At certain altitudes, from 3000 to 4000 meters, disagreeable symptoms are experienced by many persons, especially after muscular effort, which are designated usually under the term mountain sickness. The individual so affected suffers from head- ache, nausea, vertigo, great weakness, etc. Much investigation, especially of recent years, has been devoted to this subject.* Paul Bert concluded, from his numerous experiments, that a fall in baro- metric pressure acts upon the organism only in so far as there is a diminution of the partial pressure of the oxygen in the air respired. This view has been generally accepted in physiology, and mountain sickness and similar disturbances in balloon ascents have been explained, therefore, as due mainly to the lack of oxygen, — that is, to the condition of anoxemia. Mosso, on the contrary, has insisted upon the part played by the carbon dioxid. He gives experi- ments to show that there is a diminution in the carbon dioxid contents of the blood (a condition of acapnia), and it is to this, rather than to the anoxemia, that he would attribute the physio- logical results of low barometric pressures. Other authors lay stress upon the mechanical disturbances of the lung circulation, while still others assume that certain vaguely understood cosmical influences — such as the electrical condition of the air, its ioniza- tion, or radiations of some kind — may affect the metabolisms of the body and thus produce the symptoms in question. It would seem that the whole matter is more complex than was at first supposed. At a height of 4000 meters, at which mountain sick- ness is apt to occur, the barometric pressure is 460 nuns., so that there is an oxygen pressure of 92 nuns., — a pressure high enough, one would suppose, not to endanger the oxygen supply. Mosso states, also, from experiments upon monkeys, that lowering the barometric pressure sufficiently (to about 250 mms.) causes un- consciousness (sleep) even when the partial pressure of the oxygen is kept normal. The historical incident of the death of Sivel and Croce-Spinelli at an altitude of 8600 meters (barometric pressure, 262 mms.; oxygen pressure, 52.4 mms.) seems to indicate also that something more than mere diminution in oxygen pressure is respon- sible for the effects of extremely high altitudes. The incidents connected with the ascent in the balloon Zenith of Sivel, Croce-Spinelli, and Tissandier, April 15, 1875, are described in detail by the last named in "La Nature," 1875, p. 337, also in Bert's "La pression *baro- metrique," p. 1061. Only Tissandier survived. The balloonists were pro- vided with bags containing oxygen (72 percent.), but they were unable to make satisfactory use of it, since shortly after passing 7500 meters they be- came so weak that the effort to raise the arm to seize the oxygen tube was * See Kronecker, " Die Bergkrankheit," Berlin, 1903. Mosso and Marro, " Archives italiennes de biologie, " 39, 387, also vols. 40 and 41. Cohnheim, article on " Alpinismus, " " Ergebnisse der Physiologie, " vol. ii, part i, 1903. INFLUENCE OF VARIOUS CONDITIONS ON RESPIRATION. 631 impossible. Tissandier's graphic description relates that at 8000 meters it was impossible for him to speak, and that shortly afterward he became entirely unconscious. None of the three seems to have shown any signs of the violent dyspnea that precedes asphyxia caused by lack of oxygen. It is noteworthy, however, that the heart beats were very rapid, and that they experienced at first great depression of muscular strength without loss of consciousness. The onset of complete unconsciousness was sudden, but was preceded by feelings of sleepiness, which, however, were not associated with any distress. These latter facts recall the conditions of " shock, " and would suggest that probably the rapid heart beat was an indication of a great fall in blood-pressure, which may have been directly responsible for the mus- cular weakness and final unconsciousness and death. The Respiratory Quotient and its Variations. — In studying the gaseous exchanges of respiration one may determine the varia- tions in the oxygen absorbed under different conditions or in the carbon dioxid eliminated, or finally in the ratio of one to the other, ^, which is known as the respiratory quotient. In short-lasting experiments the respiratory quotient is not a very reliable indicator of the extent or character of the physiological oxidations in the body, since any alteration in the depth or rapidity of the respiratory movements may, by changing the ventilation of the alveoli, make a difference in the output of C02, — a difference, however, which would have no significance in regard to the nutritive changes of the body. In longer experiments and in those during which the respira- tory movements are not altered the determination of this ratio throws light upon the character of the oxidations that are taking place, as will be apparent from the following considerations : Under ordinary conditions of rest and upon a mixed diet the R. Q. varies between 0.65 and 0.95 (Loewy) or between 0.75 and 0.89 (Magnus Levy). If, however, the material oxidized in the body is entirely carbohydrate the R. Q. should be equal to unity: ~=^ =1. All the oxygen used in the combustion might be considered as uniting with the C to form C02, since enough O is present in the sugar to account for that used in oxidizing the H to H20. Or, as expressed in a reaction, Dextrose C6H1206 + 602 = 6C02 + 6H20. R. Q. = f = 1. The number of molecules of C02 formed in the oxidation is equal to the number of molecules of 02 used. If fats alone are oxidized in the body the R. Q. should be low (0.7), since these substances are poor in oxygen compared with the amount of C and H present in the molecule. The combustion of palmitin may be represented as follows : Palmitin, C3H5(C16H3102)3 = C^H^CV 2(CS1H9806) + 14502 = 102CO2 + 98H20. R. Q- = m = 0.7. 632 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. In the same way it may be estimated that the R. Q. for the oxidation of proteids alone is equal to 0.78. In accordance with these conclusions it is found practically that the respiratory quotient may be raised to 1, approximately at least, by feeding exclusively upon carbohydrate foods, while an excess of proteid or carbohydrate food lowers it to 0.7. In con- nection with other data, therefore, the R. Q. may be used to throw light upon the character of the nutrition. Under certain special conditions the respiratory quotient may exceed unity or fall distinctly below 0.7. A rise to a value over unity may occur temporarily because of increased ventilation of the alveoli. Deeper and more rapid breathing will drive out some of the C02 in the air of the lungs and thus increase greatly the R. Q. As previously stated, this increase has in itself no nutritional significance, but it is Fig. 252.— Record showing typical Cheyne-Stokes respiration (from a case of aortic and mitral insufficiency with arteriosclerosis). The time record gives seconds. a factor that must be allowed for in such experiments. A more suggestive increase of the R. Q. is observed during convalescence. In this period, as is well known, an individual may increase in weight rapidly, chiefly from the laying on of fat. This fat is made in large part probably from the carbohydrate of the food. An oxygen-rich food, therefore, is converted to an oxygen poor one, so that some of the oxygen must be split off partly as carbon dioxid, and there is a larger output of this substance in the expired air. Under many conditions of life — muscular exercise, for example — in which the oxidations of the body are greatly increased, the larger production of C02 is balanced by a larger absorption of O. It is interesting to find that usually this balance is so well maintained that the R. Q. does not vary sensibly. Modified Respiratory Movements. — Laughing, coughing, yawning, sneezing, sobbing, and even vomiting may be considered INFLUENCE OF VARIOUS CONDITIONS ON RESPIRATION. 633 as modified respiratory movements, since the same group of muscles come into play. These are all movements, with the exception of yawning, which may be regarded as reflexes that have nothing to do directly with the processes of respiration. A most interesting variation of the normal type of respiration is known as the Cheyne- Stokes respiration. It occurs in certain pathological conditions, such as arteriosclerosis, uremic states, fatty degeneration of the heart, etc. It is characterized by the fact that the respiratory movements occur in groups (10 to 30) separated by apneic pauses, which may last for a number (30 to 40) of seconds. After each pause the respirations begin with a small movement, gradually increase to a maximum, and then fall off gradually to the point of complete cessation (see Fig. 252). The cause of this rhythm has not been discovered. It is not certain whether the apnea between the groups of respiration is a true apnea or an inhibitory apnea or an apnea of some other kind resulting from some different kind of action upon the respiratory center. A similar rhythm is often observed in the beats of an isolated heart of the cold-blooded animals under conditions which imply an insufficient supply of oxygen. More or less rhythmical variations in the strength of the breathing movements have been described also in normal sleep, hibernation, chloral narcosis, etc., but nothing so definite and characteristic as in these very interesting Cheyne-Stokes cases. SECTION VII. PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. CHAPTER XXXIX. MOVEMENTS OF THE ALIMENTARY CANAL- Mastication. — Mastication is an entirely voluntary act. . The articulation of the mandibles with the skull permits a variety of movements; the jaw may be raised and lowered, may be projected and retracted, or may be moved from side to side, or various com- binations of these different directions of movement may be effected. The muscles concerned in these movements and their innervation are described as follows: The masseter, temporal, and internal pterygoids raise the jaw; these muscles are innervated through the inferior maxillary division of the trigeminal. The jaw is depressed mainly by the action of the digastric muscle, assisted in some cases by the mylohyoid and the geniohyoid. The two former receive motor fibers from the inferior maxillary division of the fifth cranial, the last from a branch of the hypoglossal. The lateral movements of the jaws are produced by the external pterygoids, when acting separately. Simultaneous contraction of these muscles on both sides causes projection of the lower jaw. In this latter case forcible retraction of the jaw is produced by the contraction of a part of the temporal muscle. The external pterygoids also receive their motor fibers from the fifth cranial nerve, through its inferior maxillary division. The grinding movements commonly used in masticating the food between the molar teeth are produced by a combination of the action of the external pteryogids, the elevators, and perhaps the depressors. At the same time the movements of the tongue and of the muscles of the cheeks and lips serve to keep the food properly placed for the action of the teeth, and to gather it into position for the act of swallowing. Deglutition. — The act of swallowing is a complicated reflex movement which may be initiated voluntarily, but is, for the most part, completed quite independently of the will. The classical description of the act given by Magendie divides it into three stages, 634 MOVEMENTS OF THE ALIMENTARY CANAL. 635 corresponding to the three anatomical regions — mouth, pharynx, and esophagus — through which the swallowed morsel passes on its way to the stomach. The first stage consists in the passage of the bolus of food through the isthmus of the fauces, — that is, the opening lying between the ridges formed by the palatoglossi muscles, the so-called anterior pillars of the fauces. This part of the act is usually ascribed to the movements of the tongue itself. The bolus of food lying upon its upper surface is forced backward by the ele- vation of the tongue against the soft palate from the tip toward the base. This portion of the movement may be regarded as vol- untary, to the extent at least of manipulating the food into its proper position on the dorsum of the tongue, although it is open to doubt whether the entire movement is usually effected by a voluntary act. Under normal conditions the presence of moist food upon the tongue seems essential to the complete execution of the act ; and an attempt to make the movement with very dry material upon the tongue is either not successful or is performed with difficulty. The second act comprises the passage of the bolus from the isthmus of the fauces to the esophagus, — that is, its transit through the pharynx. The pharynx being a common passage for the air and the food, it is important that this part of the act should be consummated quickly. According to the older description, the motor power driving the bolus downward through the pharynx is derived from the contrac- tion of the pharyngeal muscles, particularly the constrictors, which contract from above downward and drive the food into the esopha- gus. Kronecker and Meltzer,* however, have shown that the con- traction of the mylohyoid muscle in the floor of the mouth is the most important factor in this act of shooting the food suddenly through the pharynx into the esophagus. The contraction of this muscle marks the beginning of the purely involuntary part of the act of swallowing. The bolus of food lies upon the dorsum of the tongue and by the pressure of the front of the tongue against the hard palate it is shut off from the front part of the mouth cavity. When the mylohyoids contract sharply the bolus is put under pres- sure and is shot into and through the pharynx. This effect is aided by the contraction of the hyoglossi muscles, which by moving the tongue backward and downward tend to increase the pressure put upon the food. Simultaneously, a number of other muscles are brought into action, the general effect of which is to shut off the nasal and laryngeal openings and thus prevent the entrance of * Kronecker and Meltzer, " Archiv f . Physiologie, " 1883, suppl. volume, p. 328; also "Journal of Experimental Medicine, " 2, 453, 1897. For later work, consult Cannon and Moser, "American Journal of Physiology," 1, 435, 1898; Schreiber, "Archiv f. exper. Pathol, u. Pharmakologie, " 46, 414, 1901; and Eykman, "Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie," 99, 513, 1903. 636 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. food into the corresponding cavities. The whole reflex is there* fore an excellent example of a finely co-ordinated movement. The following events are described: The mouth cavity is shut off by the position of the tongue against the palate and by the con- traction of the muscles of the anterior pillars of the fauces. The opening into the nasal cavity is closed by the elevation of the soft palate (action of the levator palati and tensor palati muscles) and the contraction of the posterior pillars of the fauces (palatopharyn- geal muscles) and the elevation of the uvula (azygos uvulae muscle). The soft palate, uvula, and posterior pillars thus form a sloping surface shutting off the nasal chamber and facilitating the passage of the food backward through the pharynx. The respiratory opening into the larynx is closed by the adduction of the vocal cords (lateral crico-arytenoids and constrictors of the glottis) and by the strong elevation of the entire larynx and a depression of the epiglottis over the larynx (action of the thyrohyoids, digastrics, geniohyoids, and mylohyoids and the muscles in the aryteno-epiglottidean folds). If the elevation of the larynx be prevented by fixation of the thy- roid the act of swallowing becomes impossible. There is also at this time, apparently as a regular part of the swallowing reflex, a slight inspiratory movement of the diaphragm, the so-called swallowing respiration. The movements of the epiglottis during this stage of swallowing have been much discussed. The usual view is that it is pressed down upon the laryngeal orifice like the lid of a box and thus effectually protects the respiratory passage. It has been shown, however, that removal of the epiglottis does not prevent normal swallowing, and Stuart and McCormick* have reported the case of a man in whom part of the pharynx had been permanently removed by surgical operation and in whom the epiglottis could be seen during the act of swallowing. In this individual, according to their observations, the epiglottis was not folded back during swallowing, but remained erect. Kanthack and Anderson f state that in normal individuals the movement of the epiglottis backward during swallowing may be felt by simply passing the finger back into the pharynx until it comes into contact with the epiglottis. According to most observers, it is not necessary for the protection of the larynx that the epiglottis shall be actually folded down over it by the contraction of its own muscles. The forcible lifting of the larynx, together with the descent of the base of the tongue, effects the same result by mechanically crowding the parts together, and the larynx is still further guarded by the ap- proximation of the false and true vocal cords, thus closing the glottis. The whole act is very rapid as well as complex, so that not more * "Journal of Anatomy and Physiology," 1892. t "Journal of Physiology," 14, 154, 1893. MOVEMENTS OF THE ALIMENTARY CANAL. 637 than a second elapses between the beginning of the contraction of the mylohyoids and the entrance of the food into the upper end of the esophagus. The passage of the food through the esophagus differs apparently with its consistency. When the food is liquid or very soft Kronecker and Meltzer have shown that it is shot through the whole length of the esophagus by the force of the initial act of swallowing. It arrives at the lower end of the esophagus in about 0.1 sec, and may pass immediately into the stomach or may lie some moments in the esophagus according to the conditions of the sphincter guarding the cardiac orifice. When, however, the food is solid or semi- solid, as was shown by Cannon and Moser, it is forced down the esophagus by a peristaltic movement of the musculature. The circular muscles are constricted from above downward by an ad- vancing muscular wave, while the longitudinal muscles contract probably somewhat in advance of this wave so as to dilate the tube and facilitate the passage of the bolus. The upper portion of the esophagus contains cross-striated fibers indicating rapid contraction ; the lower end consists of plain muscle only, while the intermediate portion is a mixture of the two varieties. Kronecker and Meltzer believe that each of these segments contracts as a whole and in orderly succession, but other observers, on the evidence furnished by Roentgen-ray photographs, agree that there is no perceptible pause in the downward movement of the wave of contraction. These same movements occur in the swallowing of liquid or soft food, but in such cases the peristaltic wave follows the actual descent of the food. According to the observation of Kronecker and Meltzer, it takes about 6 sec. for the peristaltic wave to reach the stomach, and the passage of the food through the cardia takes place with sufficient energy to give rise to a murmur that may be heard by auscultating over this region. In the case of the more liquid food that is shot at once to the lower end of the stomach within 0.1 sec, it may apparently pass at once into the stomach or it may lie in the lower end of the esophagus until the wave of contraction reaches it (6 sec.) and forces it through the opening. At this opening, the cardia or cardiac orifice, the circular layer of muscles acts as a sphincter which is normally in a condition of tone, particularly when the stomach contains food. The advancing wave of con- traction in the esophagus either forces the food through the resis- tance offered by this sphincter or probably the sphincter suffers an inhibition at this moment as a part of the general reflex action. Kronecker and Meltzer have noted the interesting fact that if a second swallow is made within an interval of six seconds after the first, the peristaltic wave occasioned by the latter is inhibited at whatever portion of its path it may have reached. The food carried 638 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. down by the first swallow waits in this case for the arrival of the suc- ceeding wave before entering the stomach. Nervous Control of Deglutition. — The entire act of swallowing, as has been said, is essentially a reflex act. Even the comparatively simple wave of contraction that sweeps over the esophagus is due to a reflex nervous stimulation, and is not a simple conduction of contraction from one portion of the tube to another. This fact was demonstrated by the experiments of Mosso,* who found that after removal of an entire segment from the esophagus the peristaltic wave passed in due time to the portion of the esophagus left on the stomach side, in spite of the anatomical break. The same experi- ment was performed successfully on rabbits by Kronecker and Meltzer. Observation of the stomach end of the esophagus in this animal showed that it went into contraction two seconds after the beginning of a swallowing act whether the esophagus was intact or ligated or completely divided by a transverse incision. The afferent nerves concerned in this reflex are the sensory fibers to the mucous membrane of the pharynx and esophagus, including branches of the glossopharyngeal, trigeminal, vagus, and superior laryngeal division of the vagus. Artificial stimulation of this last nerve in the lower animals is known to produce swallowing movements. Several observers have attempted to determine the precise area or areas in the pharyngeal membrane from which the sensory impulses liberating the reflex normally start. According to Kahn,f the most effective areas from whose stimulation the reflex may be produced vary in location in different animals. In the rabbit the reflex is originated most easily by stimulation at the entrance to the pharynx — the soft palate — along the line extending from the posterior edge of the hard palate to the tonsils (superior maxil- lary branch of trigeminal); in the dog irritation of the posterior pharyngeal wall is most effective (glossopharyngeal nerve); in monkeys the area is approximately as in rabbits, — that is, in the region of the tonsils. The motor fibers concerned in the reflex comprise the hypoglossal, the trigeminal, the glossopharyngeal, the vagus, and the spinal accessory. For an act of such complexity and such perfect co-ordination it has been assumed that there is a special nerve center, the swallowing or deglutition center, which has been located in the medulla at the level of the origin of the vagi. There is little positive knowledge, however, concerning the existence of this center as a definite group of intermediary nerve cells, after the type of the vasoconstrictor or respiratory center, which send their axons to the motor nuclei of the several efferent nerves con- cerned. As in the case of other complicated reflex acts, we can only * Moleschott's "Untersuchungen," 1876, volume xi. t Kahn, " Archiv f. Physiologie, " 1903, suppl. volume, 386. MOVEMENTS OF THE ALIMENTARY CANAL. 639 say that the deglutition reflex is controlled by a definite nervous mechanism the final motor cells of which are scattered in the several motor nuclei of the efferent nerves mentioned above. The Anatomy of the Stomach. — The stomach in man belongs to the simple type as distinguished from the compound stomachs of some of the other mammalia, — the ruminating animals, for example. Physiological and histological investigations have shown, however, that the so-called simple stomachs are divided into parts that have different properties and functions. The names and bound- aries of these parts can not be stated precisely, since they vary in different animals, and moreover there is at present an unfortunate want of agreement among different authors regarding the nomen- clature of the parts of the stomach.* For the purposes of a physio- logical description we may use the names indicated in the accom- panying schematic figure. The main interest lies in the separation num., J-Pyiorus- Pyloric part of stomach or antrum. jbyiori. Position, of traitsuersebcmcL Intermediate or jorebylorie regioru Fig. 253. — Schematic figure to show the different parts of the stomach. — (After Retzius.) of the pyloric part of the stomach or antrum pylorif from the main cavity of the stomach. The line of separation is marked by a fissure on the small curvature, incisura angularis (/. A.) and on the large curvature by an abrupt change of direction. The pyloric part makes an angle, therefore, with the body of the stomach and differs from the latter in its musculature, the macroscopical and microscopi- cal characteristics of its mucous membrane, and in its functional importance. The main body of the stomach falls into two sub- divisions, whose line of demarcation is, however, indefinite. The fundus proper is the blind, rounded end of the stomach to the left of the cardia and projecting toward the spleen. The intermediate or prepyloric region shows in many animals a characteristic struc- * See His, " Archiv f . Anatomie, "1903, p. 345. t Some recent writers confine this term antrum pylori to that portion of the pyloric region bordering upon the pyloric orifice. 640 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. ture in its secreting glands. It is in this region that the hydro- chloric acid of the gastric juice is mainly secreted. The Musculature of the Stomach. — The musculature of the stomach is usually divided into three layers, — a longitudinal, an oblique, and a circular coat. The longitudinal coat is continuous at the cardia with the longitudinal fibers of the esophagus ; it spreads out from this point along the length of the stomach, forming a layer of varying thickness; along the curvatures the layer is stronger than on the front and posterior surfaces, while at the pyloric end it increases considerably in thickness, and passes over the pylorus to be continued directly into the longitudinal coat of the duodenum. The layer of oblique fibers is quite incomplete; it seems to be continuous with the circular fibers of the esophagus, and spreads out from the cardia for a certain distance over the front and posterior surfaces of the fundus of the stomach, but toward the pyloric end disappears, seeming to pass into the circular fibers. The circular coat, which is placed between the two preceding layers, is the thick- est and most important part of the musculature of the stomach. At the fundus the circular bands are thin and somewhat loosely placed, but toward the pyloric end they increase much in thickness, forming a strong, muscular mass, which, as we shall see, plays the most important part in the movements of the stomach. At the pylorus itself a special development of this layer functions as a sphincter pylori, which with the aid of a circular fold of the mucous membrane makes it possible to shut off the duodenum completely from the cavity of the stomach. The line of separation between the antrum pylori and the body of the stomach is made by a special thickening of the circular fibers which forms a structure known as the "transverse band" by the older writers,* and de- scribed more recentlyf as the "sphincter antri pylorici." The Movements of the Stomach. — The solid food remains in the stomach for several hours, and during this time the musculature contracts in such a way that the thinner portions as they are formed by digestion are ejected from time to time through the pylorus into the intestine. Except at the definite intervals when the pyloric sphincter relaxes the food is entirely shut off from the rest of the alimentary canal by the tonic closure of the sphincters at the cardia and the pylorus. There is a certain orderliness in the movements of the stomach, and especially in the separation and ejection of the more liquid from the solid parts, which shows the existence of a specially adapted mechanism. These movements have been studied by many investigators, making use of various experimental meth- * See Beaumont, " Physiology of Digestion, " second edition, 1847, p. 104. t Hofmeister und Schutz, " Archiv f . exper. Pathologie und Pharmakol- ogie, " 1886, vol. xx. MOVEMENTS OF THE ALIMENTARY CANAL. 641 ods. The first noteworthy contributions to this subject were those made in this country by Beaumont in his famous observations upon Alexis St. Martin, the Canadian voyageur, who had a per- manent fistulous opening in his stomach as the result of a gunshot wound.* In recent years the subject has been studied with great success by means of the X-rays,f on the excised stomach, J and by means of tambours or sounds introduced into the stomach to meas- ure the pressure changes. § These researches all unite in em- phasizing one fundamental point, — namely, that the fundic end of the stomach is not actively concerned in these movements, but serves rather as a reservoir for retaining the bulk of the food, while the muscular pyloric region is the apparatus which triturates and macerates the food and forces it out from time to time into the duodenum. According to the observations made with the X-ray apparatus, movements begin a few minutes after the entrance of food into the stomach. Small contractions start in the middle region of the stomach and run toward the pylorus. These moving waves of contraction appear at regular intervals. The pyloric portion becomes lengthened and it may be noticed that in this region the peristaltic waves become more and more forcible as digestion progresses. These running waves of contraction serve to press the stomach contents against the pylorus. According to Cannon, they occur in the cat at intervals of 10 sec. and each wave requires about 20 sec. to reach the pylorus. According to Beaumont's observations, the contraction waves follow at longer intervals in man (2 to 3 min.). The obvious result of these movements is to mix the food thoroughly in the intermediate and pyloric portions of the stomach with the acid gastric juice and to reduce it to a thin, liquid mass, — the chyme. At certain intervals the pyloric sphincter relaxes and the contraction wave squeezes some of the fluid contents into the duodenum with considerable force. The mechanism controlling the relaxation of this sphincter is obscure. It does not occur with the ap- proach of each contraction wave, but at irregular intervals. Cannon connects it with the consistency of the food. Solid objects forced against the pylorus prevent relaxation and retard the passage of the chyme into the intestine. Wheh liquid food alone is taken into the stomach numerous observations made by means of intestinal fistulas show that the material is forced into the duodenum within a few minutes. According to this description, * See Osier, "Journal of the American Medical Association," Nov. 15, 1902, for life of Beaumont and account of his work. t See Cannon, "American Journal of Physiology," 1, 359, 1898; and Roux and Balthazard, "Archives de Physiologie, " 10, 85, 1898. % Hofmeister and Schiitz, loc. cit. § Moritz, " Zeitschrift f . Biologie, " 32, 359, 1895. 41 642 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. the portion of the food toward the pyloric end of the stomach is the first to be thoroughly mixed with the gastric juice, and to be broken down partly by digestion and partly by the mechanical action of the contractions. This portion as it is liquefied is expelled and its place is taken by new material forced forward from the fundic end. It would seem that this portion of the stomach is in a condition of tone, and the pressure thus put upon the contents is sufficient to force them slowly toward the pyloric end as this be- comes emptied. The older view was that the contents of the stomach are kept in a general rotary movement so as to become more or less uniformly mixed; but Cannon's observations, and especially those of Grutzner, * indicate that the material at the fundic end may remain undisturbed for a long time and thus escape mixture with the acid gastric juice. This fact is of impor- tance in connection with the salivary digestion of the starchy foods. Obviously salivary digestion may proceed for a long time without being affected by the acid of the stomach. Grutzner fed rats with food of different colors and found that the successive portions were arranged in definite strata. The food first taken lay next to the rtoma-of^rS^d&tiSnS walls of the stomach, while the S0Jiffehreenttr S^-(Sr&^ gThe succeeding portions were arranged food was given m three portions and regularly in the interior in a COn- colored differently: first, black; sec- . „ , . . , ond, white (indicated by vertical centric fashion, as shown in the marking) ; third, red (indicated by n ^ , » transverse marking). figure. Such an arrangement of the food is more readily understood when one recalls that the stomach has never any empty space within; its cavity is only as large as its contents, so that the first portion of food eaten entirely fills it and successive portions find the wall layer occupied and are therefore received into the interior. The ingestion of much liquid must interfere somewhat with this stratification. Whether the fact of this stratification has any hygienic bearing with regard to the most desirable sequence in our articles of diet is not yet apparent. Cannonf has reported some interesting experiments upon the relative duration of gastric digestion for carbohydrates, proteids, and fats when fed separately and combined. The foods were mixed with subnitrate of bismuth and their position in the stomach and passage into the intestine were watched by means of the Roentgen rays. It was found that * Grutzner, " Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 106, 463, 1905. t Cannon, "American Journal of Physiology, " 12, 387, 1904. MOVEMENTS OF THE ALIMENTARY CANAL. 643 carbohydrate food begins to pass out from the stomach soon after ingestion, and requires only about one half as much time as the pro- teids for complete gastric digestion. Fats remain long in the stomach when taken alone, and when combined with the other foodstuffs markedly delay their exit through the pylorus. This distinct difference in the main foodstuffs can hardly be referred to mere mechanical consistency, since the fats are liquefied by the heat of the body. There is an indication, therefore, that the relaxation of the sphincter may be controlled in some way by chemical stimuli originating in the digested food. Regarding the general mechanism of the stomach, it may be pointed out that it forms an admirably adapted apparatus for receiving at once or within a short period a large amount of food which it reduces to a liquid or semiliquid condition, partly by digestion, partly mechanically, and that it charges the intestine at intervals with small amounts of this chyme in such a condition as to admit of rapid digestion. It seems obvious that without the stomach our mode of eating would have to be changed, as it would not be possible to load the intestine rapidly with a large supply of food such as is consumed at an ordinary meal. The Relation of the Nerves to the Movements of the Stomach. — The stomach receives nerve fibers from two sources, — the vagi and the splanchnics, — but it seems probable that its orderly movements may be merely regulated through these extrinsic fibers, and that it is essentially an automatic organ. Thus, it has been shown that the excised stomach (Hofmeister and Schutz) when kept warm continues to execute regular movements which, if not identical with those observed under normal conditions, have at least an orderly sequence. So also it would appear from the results of the older observers * that gastric digestion may proceed normally both as regards secretion and movements after section of the extrinsic nerves. The point has not yet perhaps been demonstrated conclusively, but provisionally we may regard the stomach, con- sidered as a motor mechanism, as an automatic organ like the heart. Its stimuli to movement arise within itself, but these movements are regulated by the action of the extrinsic nerve fibers so as to adapt them to varying conditions. Whether the automaticity is a property of the plain muscle tissue itself, or depends upon the rich supply of intrinsic nerve ganglia (plexuses of Meissner and Auer- bach), is a question that can not be answered at present. The extrinsic nerves not only supply the stomach with efferent fibers, motor and secretory, but also carry afferent fibers from the stomach to the central nervous system. Regarding the purely efferent * See Heidenhain in Hermann's " Handbuch der Physiologie, " vol. v, p. 118. 644 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. action of the extrinsic nerves, the results of numerous experiments seem to show quite conclusively that in general the fibers received along the vagus path are motor, artificial stimulation of them caus- ing more or less well marked contractions of part or all of the musculature of the stomach. It has been shown that the sphincter pylori as well as the rest of the musculature is supplied by motor fibers from these nerves. The fibers coming through the splanch- nics, on the contrary, are mainly inhibitory. When stimulated they cause a dilatation of the contracted stomach and a relaxation of the sphincter pylori. Some observers have reported experiments which seem to show that this anatomical separation of the motor and inhibitory fibers is not complete; that some inhibitory fibers may be found in the vagi and some motor fibers in the splanchnics. The anatomical courses of these fibers are insufficiently known, but there seems to be no question as to the existence of the two physio- logical varieties. Through their activity, without doubt, the move- ments of the stomach may be influenced, favorably or unfavorably, by conditions directly or indirectly affecting the central nervous system. Wertheimer* has shown experimentally that stimulation of the central end of the sciatic or the vagus nerve may cause reflex inhibition of the tonus of the stomach, and Doyonf has confirmed this result in cases in which the movements and tonicity of the stom- ach were first increased by the action of pilocarpin and strychnin. Cannon, in his observations upon cats, found that all movements of the stomach ceased as soon as the animal showed signs of anxiety, rage, or distress. Movements of the Intestines. — The muscles of the small and the large intestine are arranged in two layers, — an outer longitudinal and an inner circular coat, — while between these coats and in the submucous coat there are present the nerve-plexuses of Auerbach and Meissner. The general arrangement of muscles and nerves is similar, therefore, to that prevailing in the stomach, and in accor- dance with this we find that the physiological activities exhibited are of much the same character, only, perhaps, not quite so complex. Two main forms of intestinal movement have been distinguished, — the peristaltic and the pendular. Peristalsis. — The peristaltic movement consists in a constriction of the walls of the intestine, which, beginning at a certain point, passes downward away from the stomach, from segment to segment, while the parts behind the advancing zone of constriction gradually relax. The evident effect of such a movement is to push onward the contents of the intestines in the direction of the movement. It is obvious that the circular layer of muscles is chiefly involved in * "Archives de physiologic normale et pathologique, " 1892, p. 379. f Ibid., 1895, p. 374. MOVEMENTS OF THE ALIMENTARY CANAL. 645 peristalsis, since constriction can only be produced by contraction of this layer. To what extent the longitudinal muscles enter into the movement is not definitely determined. The term "anti- peristalsis " is used to describe the same form of movement running in the opposite direction — that is, toward the stomach. Anti- peristalsis is said not to occur under normal conditions; it has been observed sometimes in isolated pieces of intestine or in the exposed intestine of living animals when stimulated artificially, and Griitz- ner* reports a number of curious experiments which seem to show that substances such as hairs, animal charcoal, etc., introduced into the rectum may travel upward to the stomach under certain con- ditions. The peristaltic wave normally passes downward, and that this direction of movement is dependent upon some definite ar- rangement in the intestinal walls is shown by the experiments of Mallf upon reversal of the intestines. In these experiments a portion of the small intestine was resected, turned around, and sutured in place again; so that in this piece what was the lower end became the upper end. In those animals that made a good recovery the nutritive condition gradually became very serious, and when the animals were killed and examined it was found that there was an accumulation of food at the stomach end of the reversed piece of intestine, and that this region showed marked dilatation. The peristaltic movements of the intestines may be observed upon living animals when the abdomen is opened. If the operation is made in the air and the intestines are exposed to its influence, or if the conditions of temperature and circulation are otherwise disturbed, the movements observed are often violent and irregular. The peristalsis runs rapidly along the intestines and may pass over the whole length in about a minute; at the same time the con- traction of the longitudinal muscles gives the bowels a peculiar writhing movement. Movements of this kind are evidently abnormal, and only occur in the body under the strong stimulation of pathological conditions. Normal peristalsis, the object of which is to move the food slowly along the alimentary tract, is quite a different affair. Observers all agree that the wave of contraction is gentle and progresses slowly, although at different rates perhaps in different parts of the intestine. According to Bayliss and Star- ling,:]: the peristaltic movement is a complicated reflex through the intrinsic ganglia. When the intestine is stimulated by a bolus placed within its cavity, the musculature above the point stimulated is excited, while that below is inhibited. In accordance with this law they find that in peristalsis the advancing wave of constriction *" Deutsche medicinische Wochenschrift, " No. 48, 1894. t" Johns Hopkins Hospital Reports," 1, 93, 1896. % "Journal of Physiology," 24, 99, 1899. if f'^-Wl PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. Kit la r i^tereceded by a wave of relaxation or inhibition. The force of the $ fiSjifbraction as measured by Cash * in the dog's intestine is very small, eight of five to eight grams was sufficient to check the onward ^m/vement of the substance in the intestine and to set up violent, ^Joolicky contractions which caused the animal evident uneasiness. / f§>s^e may suPPose that under normal conditions each injection of //chyme from the stomach into the duodenum is followed by a peristalsis that, beginning at the duodenum, passes slowly down- ward over a part or all of the small intestine. According to most observers, the movement is blocked at the ileocecal valve, and the peristaltic movements of the large intestine form an independent group. Mechanism of the Peristaltic Movement. — The means by which the peristaltic movement makes its orderly forward progression have not been determined beyond question. The simplest explana- tion would be to assume that an impulse is conveyed directly from cell to cell in the circular muscular coat, so that a contraction started at any point would spread by direct conduction of the contraction change. This theory, however, does not explain satisfactorily the normal conduction of the wave of contraction always in one direc- tion, nor the fact that the wave of contraction is preceded by a wave of inhibition. Moreover, Bayliss and Starling state that, although the peristaltic movements continue after section of the extrinsic nerves, — indeed, become more marked under these con- ditions,— the application of cocain or nicotin prevents their oc- currence. Since these substances may be supposed to act on the intrinsic nerves, it is probable that the co-ordination of the move- ment is effected through the local nerve ganglia, but our knowledge of the mechanism and physiology of these peripheral nerve-plexuses is as yet quite incomplete. Pendular Movements. — In addition to the peristaltic wave a second kind of movement may be observed in the exposed intestines of a living animal. This movement is characterized by a gentle swinging to and fro of the different loops, whence its name of pendu- lar movement. Mallf has shown that the main feature of this movement is a rhythmical contraction of the circular muscles. He prefers to speak of the movements as rhythmical instead of pendular contractions, and points out that, owing to the arrange- ment of the blood-vessels in the coats of the intestine, the rhythmical contractions should act as a pump to expel the blood from the submucous venous plexus into the radicles of the superior mesenteric vein, and thus materially aid in keeping up the circulation through the intestine and in maintaining a good pressure in the portal vein, *" Proceedings of the Roval Society," London, 41, 1887. t" Johns Hopkins Hospital Reports," 1, 37, 1896. MOVEMENTS OF THE ALIMENTARY CANAL. in much the same way as happens in the case of the spleen. Bay i£ 3 and Starling corroborate this view, except that they find that b\t the circular and longitudinal layers of muscle are concerned in movement. The rhythmical contractions, according to theVe' observers, are entirely muscular in origin, since they persist afteV 7^ the application of nicotin or cocain. Vfi Cannon* has studied the movements of the small intestines most successfully by means of the Roentgen rays. He finds, in the cat, that the most characteristic phenomenon is that due to the rhyth- mical or pendular movements. By means of these contractions masses or strings of the food are suddenly segmented repeatedly and in a definite manner into a number of small pieces, which move to and fro as the pieces combine and are again separated. These segmentations may proceed at the rate of thirty per minute for a certain time, and the apparent result is that the material is well mixed with the digestive secretions and is brought thoroughly into contact with the absorptive walls. During these rhythmical contractions there is no steady progression of the food; it remains in the same region, although subjected to repeated divisions. From time to time the separated pieces are caught, as it were, by an advancing peristaltic wave, moved forward a certain dis- tance, and gathered again into a new mass. In this new location the rhythmical contractions again segment and churn the mass before a new peristaltic wave moves it on. According to this description, the rhythmical movements are local contractions (mainly of the circular muscles) which seem to be due to the local distension caused by the food. They occur rhythmically for a certain period and then cease until a new series is started. Some- what similar movements have been described by Bunch f from observations on the isolated intestine. The curious observation is reported J that during the period of fasting (dog) the whole gastro- intestinal canal, although empty, shows at intervals rhythmical contractions of its musculature which may last for twenty to thirty minutes (see p. 703). The Nervous Control of the Intestinal Movements. — As stated, there is some evidence to show that the rhythmical con- tractions of the intestines are muscular in origin (myogenic), while the more co-ordinated peristaltic movements depend upon the intrinsic nervous mechanism. The intestine is, however, not de- pendent for either movement upon its connections with the central nervous system. Like the stomach, it is an automatic organ whose activity is regulated through its extrinsic nerves. * Cannon, "American Journal of Physiology," 6, 251, 1902. t Bunch, " Journal of Physiology," 22, 357, 1897. I Boldireff, "Archives des sciences biologiques, " 11, 1, 1905. 648 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. The small intestine and the greater part of the large intestine receive visceromotor nerve fibers from the vagi and the sympathetic chain. The former, according to most observers, when artifically stimulated cause movements of the intestine, and are therefore regarded as the motor fibers. It seems probable, however, that the vagi carry or may carry in some animals inhibitory fibers as well, and that the motor effects usually obtained upon stimulation are due to the fact that in these nerves the motor fibers predominate. The fibers received from the sympathetic chain, on the other hand, give mainly an inhibitory effect when stimulated, although some motor fibers apparently may take this path. Bechterew and Mislawski * state that the sympathetic fibers for the small intestine emerge from the spinal cord as medullated fibers in the sixth dorsal to the first lumbar spinal nerves, (or lower — Bunch) and pass to the sympathetic chain in the splanchnic nerves and thence to the semilunar plexus. The paths of these fibers through the central nervous system are not known, but there are evidently connections extending to the higher brain centers, since psychical states are known to influence the movements of the intestine, and according to some observers stimulation of portions of the cerebral cortex may produce movements or relaxation of the walls of the small and large intestines. Effect of Various Conditions upon the Intestinal Move- ments.— Experiments have shown that the movements of the in- testines may be evoked in many ways in addition to direct stimu- lation of the extrinsic nerves. Chemical stimuli may be applied directly to the intestinal wall. Mechanical stimulation — pinching, for example, or the introduction of a bolus into the intestinal cavity — may start peristaltic movements. Violent movements may be produced also by shutting off the blood-supply, and again temporarily when the supply is re-established. A condition of dyspnea may also start movements in the intestines or in some cases inhibit movements which are already in progress, the stimu- lus in this case seeming to act upon the central nervous system and to stimulate both the motor and the inhibitory fibers. Oxygen gas within the bowels tends to suspend the movements of the intes- tine, while C02, CH4, and H2S act as stimuli, increasing the move- ments. Organic acids, such as acetic, propionic, formic, and caprylic, which may be formed normally within the intestine as the result of bacterial action, act also as strong stimulants. Movements of the Large Intestine. — The musculature in the large intestine has the same general arrangement as in the small, and the usual view has been that the movements are similar, although more infrequent, so that the material received from the * " Archiv f . Physiologie, " 1889, suppl. volume. MOVEMENTS OF THE ALIMENTARY CANAL. 649 small intestine is slowly moved along while becoming more and more solid from the absorption of water, until in the form of feces it reaches the sigmoid flexure and rectum. Bayliss and Starling state that their law of intestinal peristalsis holds in this portion of the intestine, — that is, local excitation causes a constriction above and a dilatation below the point stimulated. Cannon,* however, from his studies of the normal movements in cats, as seen by the Roentgen rays, comes to the conclusion that the movements in the large intestine show a marked peculiarity previously overlooked. He divides the large intestine into two parts; in the second, cor- responding roughly to the descending colon the food is moved toward the rectum by peristaltic waves. A number of constrictions may be seen simultaneously within a length of some inches. In the ascending and transverse colon and cecum, on the contrary, the most frequent movement is that of antiperistalsis. The food in this portion of the canal is more or less liquid and its presence sets up running waves of constriction, which, beginning somewhere in the colon, pass toward the ileocecal valve. These waves occur in groups separated by periods of rest. The presence of the ileocecal valve prevents the material from being forced back into the small in- testine. The value of this peculiar reversal of the normal move- ment of the bowels at this particular point would seem to lie in the fact that it delays the passage of the material toward the rectum and by thoroughly mixing it gives increased opportunities for the completion of the processes of digestion and absorption. As the colon becomes filled some of the material penetrates into the descending part where the normal peristalsis carries it toward the rectum. The large intestine — particularly the descending colon and rectum — receives its nerve supply from two sources: (1) Fibers which leave the spinal cord in the lumbar nerves (second to fifth in cat), pass to the sympathetic chain, and thence to the inferior mesenteric ganglia, which probably forms the termination of the preganglionic fiber. From this point the path is continued by fibers running in the hypogastric nerves and plexus. Stimulation of these fibers has given different results in the hands of various observers, but the most recent work f indicates that they are inhibitory. (2) Fibers that leave the cord in the sacral nerves (second to fourth) form part of the nervi erigentes and enter into the pelvic plexus. When stimulated these fibers cause contractions of the muscular coats; they may be regarded, therefore, as motor fibers. As in the * Cannon, loc. cit. t Langley and Anderson, "Journal of Physiology," 18, 67, 1895. Bay- liss and Starling, ibid., 26, 107, 1900. Also Wischnewsky, in Hermann's " Jahresbericht der Physiologie, " vol. xii, 1905. 650 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. case of the small intestine and stomach, we may assume that these motor and inhibitory fibers serve for the reflex regulation and adaptation of the movements. Defecation. — The undigested and indigestible parts of the food, together with some of the debris and secretions from the alimentary tract eventually reach the sigmoid flexure and rectum. Here the nearly solid material stimulates by its pressure the sensory nerves of the rectum and produces a distinct sensation and desire to defecate. The fecal material is retained within the rectum by the action of the two sphincter muscles which close the anal opening. One of these muscles, the internal sphincter, is a strong band of the circular layer of involuntary muscle which forms one of the coats of the rectum. When the rectum contains fecal material this muscle seems to be thrown into a condition of tonic contraction until the act of defecation begins, when it is relaxed. The sphincter is composed of involuntary muscle and is innervated by fibers having the general course given above for the nerves of the large intestine. The external sphincter ani is composed of striated muscle tissue and is under the control of the will to a certain extent. When, however, the stimulus from the rectum is sufficiently intense, voluntary control is overcome and this sphincter is also relaxed. The act of defecation is in part voluntary and in part involuntary. The involuntary factor is found in the contractions of the strongly developed musculature of the rectum, especially the circular layer which serves to force the feces onward, and the relaxation of the internal sphincter. It would seem that these two acts are mainly caused by reflex stimulation from the lumbar spinal cord, although it is probable that the rectum, like the rest of the alimentary tract, is capable of automatic contractions. The rectal muscles receive a double nervous supply, containing physiologically both motor and inhibitory fibers. The former come probably from the nervus erigens by way of the pelvic plexus ; the latter from the lumbar cord through the corresponding sympathetic ganglia, inferior mesenteric ganglion, and hypogastric nerve. It has been asserted that stimu- lation of the nervus erigens causes contraction of the longitudinal muscles and inhibition of the circular muscles, while stimulation of the hypogastric nerve causes contraction of the circular muscles and inhibition of the longitudinal layer. This division of activity has not been confirmed by recent experiments. The voluntary factor in defecation consists in the inhibition of the external sphincter and the contraction of the abdominal muscles. When these latter muscles are contracted and at the same time the diaphragm is prevented from moving upward by the closure of the glottis, the increased abdominal pressure is brought to bear upon the abdominal and pelvic viscera, and aids strongly in pressing the MOVEMENTS OF THE ALIMENTARY CANAL. 651 contents of the descending colon and sigmoid flexure into the rectum. The pressure in the abdominal cavity is still further increased if a deep inspiration is first made and then maintained during the contraction of the abdominal muscles. Although the act of defeca- tion is normally initiated by voluntary effort, it may also be aroused by a purely involuntary reflex when the sensory stimulus is suf- ficiently strong. Goltz* has shown that in dogs in which the spinal cord had been severed in the lower thoracic region defecation was performed normally. In later experiments, in which the entire spinal cord was removed, except in the cervical .and upper part of the thoracic region, it was found that the animal, after it had re- covered from the operation, had normal movement once or twice a day, indicating that the rectum and lower bowels acted by virtue of their intrinsic mechanism. An interesting result of these experi- ments was the fact that the external sphincter suffered no atrophy, although its motor nerve was destroyed, and that it eventually regained its tonic activity. It would seem that the whole act of defecation is, at bottom, an involuntary reflex. The physiological center for the movement probably lies in the lumbar cord, and has sensory and motor con- nections with the rectum and the muscles of defecation; but this center is probably provided with connections with the centers of the cerebrum, through which the act may be controlled by volun- tary impulses and by various psychical states, the effect of emo- tions upon defecation being a matter of common knowledge. In infants the essentially involuntary character of the act is well known. Vomiting. — The act of vomiting causes an ejection of the con- tents of the stomach through the esophagus and mouth to the exterior. It was long debated whether the force producing this ejection comes from a strong contraction of the walls of the stomach itself or whether it is due mainly to the action of the walls of the abdomen. A forcible spasmodic contraction of the abdominal muscles takes place, as may easily be observed by any one upon himself, and it is now believed that the contraction of these muscles is the principal factor in vomiting. Magendie found that if the stomach was extirpated and a bladder containing water was sub- stituted in its place and connected with the esophagus, injection of an emetic caused a typical vomiting movement with ejection of the contents of the bladder. Gianuzzi showed, on the other hand, that upon a curarized animal vomiting could not be produced by an emetic — because, apparently, the muscles of the abdomen were paralyzed by the curare. There are on record a number of ob- servations which tend to show that the stomach is not passive *"Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 8, 160, 1874; 63, 362, 1896. 6o2 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. during the act. ( )n the contrary, it may exhibit contractions, more or less violent in character. According to Openchowski,* the pylorus is closed and the pyloric end of the stomach firmly con- tracted so as to drive the contents toward the dilated cardiac por- tion. Cannon states that in cats the normal peristaltic waves pass over the pyloric portion in the period preceding the vomiting and that finally a strong contraction at the "transverse band" com- pletely shuts off the pyloric portion from the body of the stomach, which at this time is quite relaxed. The act of vomiting is, in fact, a complex reflex movement into which many muscles enter. The following events are described : The vomiting is usually preceded by a sensation of nausea and a reflex flow of saliva into the mouth. These phenomena are succeeded or accompanied by retching move- ments, which consist essentially in deep, spasmodic inspirations with a closed glottis. The effect of these movements is to compress the stomach by the descent of the diaphragm, and at the same time to increase decidedly the negative pressure in the thorax, and therefore in the thoracic portion of the esophagus. During one of these retching movements the act of vomiting is effected by a convulsive contraction of the abdominal wall that exerts a sudden additional strong pressure upon the stomach. At the same time the cardiac orifice of the stomach is dilated, possibly by an inhibition of the sphincter, and according to the above description the fundic end of the stomach is also dilated, while the pyloric end is in strong contraction. The stomach contents are therefore forced violently out of the stomach through the esophagus, the negative pressure in the latter probably assisting in the act. The passage through the esophagus is effected mainly by the force of the contraction of the abdominal muscles; there is no evidence of antiperistaltic move- ments on the part of the esophagus itself. During the ejection of the contents of the stomach the glottis is kept closed by the adductor muscles, and usually the nasal chamber is likewise shut off from the pharynx by the contraction of the posterior pillars of the fauces on the palate and uvula. In violent vomiting, however, the vomited material may break through this latter barrier and be ejected partially through the nose. Nervous Mechanism of Vomiting. — That vomiting is a reflex act is abundantly shown by the frequency with which it is produced in consequence of the stimulation of sensory nerves or as the result of injuries to various parts of the central nervous system. After lesions or injuries of the brain vomiting often results. Disagreeable emotions and disturbances of the sense of equilibrium may produce the same result. Irritation of the mucous membrane of various parts of the alimentary canal (as, for example, tickling the back *"Archiv f. Physiologie, " 1889, p. 552. MOVEMENTS OF THE ALIMENTARY CANAL. 653 of the pharynx with the finger); disturbances of the urogenital apparatus, the liver, and other visceral organs; artificial stimula- tion of the trunk of the vagus and of other sensory nerves, may all cause vomiting. Under ordinary conditions, however, irritation of the sensory nerves of the gastric mucous membrane is the most common cause of vomiting. This effect may result from the prod- ucts of fermentation in the stomach in cases of indigestion, or may be produced intentionally by local emetics, such as mustard, taken into the stomach. The afferent path in this case is through the sensory fibers of the vagus. The efferent paths of the reflex are found in the motor nerves innervating the muscles concerned in the vomiting, — namely, the vagus, the phrenics, and the spinal nerves supplying the abdominal muscles. Whether or not there is a defi- nite vomiting center in which the afferent impulses are received and through which a co-ordinated series of efferent impulses is sent out to the various muscles has not been satisfactorily deter- mined. It has been shown that the portion of the nervous system through which the reflex is effected lies in the medulla, and it may be observed that the muscles concerned in the act, outside those of the stomach, are respiratory muscles. Vomiting, in fact, consists essentially in a simultaneous spasmodic contraction of expiratory (abdominal) muscles and inspiratory muscles (diaphragm). It has therefore been suggested that the reflex involves the stimulation of the respiratory center or some part of it. Thumas claims to have located a vomiting center in the medulla in the immediate neighbor- hood of the calamus scriptorius. Further evidence, however, is required upon this point. The act of vomiting may be produced not only as a reflex from various sensory nerves, but may also be caused by direct action upon the medullary centers. The action of apomorphin is most easily explained by supposing that it acts directly on the nerve centers. CHAPTER XL. GENERAL CONSIDERATIONS UPON THE COMPOSITION OF THE FOOD AND THE ACTION OF ENZYMES. Foods and Foodstuffs. — The term food when used in a popular sense includes everything that we eat for the purpose of nourishing the body. From this point of view the food of mankind is of a most varied character, comprising a great variety of products of the animal and vegetable kingdoms. Chemical analysis of the animal and vegetable foods shows, however, that they all contain one or more of five or six different classes of substances which are usually designated as the foodstuffs (older names, alimentary or proximate principles) on the belief that they form the useful constituent of our foods. The classification of foodstuffs usually given is as follows: Water. Inorganic salts. Proteids. Foodstuffs ( Albuminoids, a group of bodies resembling proteids, but hav- ing in some respects a different nutritive value. Carbohydrates. Fats. From the scientific point of view, a foodstuff or food may be defined as a substance absolutely necessary to the normal composition of the body, as in the case of water and salts, or as a substance which can be acted upon by the tissues of the body in such a way as to yield energy (heat, for example) or to furnish material for the pro- duction of living tissue. Moreover, to be a food in the physiological sense the substance must not directly or indirectly affect injuriously the normal nutritive processes of the tissues. The five or six substances named above are all foods in this sense. The water and certain salts of sodium, potassium, calcium, magnesium, iron, and perhaps other elements are absolutely necessary to maintain the normal composition of the tissue. Complete withdrawal of any one of these constituents would cause the death of the organism. Proteids, fats, and carbohydrates, on the other hand, are substances whose molecules have a more or less complex structure. When eaten and digested they enter the body liquids and are employed either in the synthesis of the more complex living matter, or they undergo various chemical changes, spoken of in general as metab- olism, which result finally in the breaking up of their complex 654 COMPOSITION OF FOOD AND ACTION OF ENZYMES. 655 molecules into simpler compounds. The chemical changes of metab- olism or nutrition are, on the whole, exothermic, — that is, they are attended by the production of heat. Some of the chemical or in- ternal energy that held the complex molecules together assumes the form of heat, or perhaps muscular work, after these molecules are broken down to simpler, more stable structures, such as water, carbon dioxid, and urea. Proteids, fats, and carbohydrates form materials that the tissue cells are adjusted to act upon after they have under- gone certain changes during digestion. Other complex organic compounds containing chemical energy are either injurious to the tissues or they have a structure such that the tissues can not act upon them. Such substances can not be considered as foods in the scien- tific sense. When, therefore, we desire to know the food value of any animal or vegetable product, we analyze it to determine its com- position as regards water, salts, proteids, fats, and carbohydrates. The following table compiled by Munk from the analyses given by Konig* may be taken as an indication of the average composition of the most commonly used foods: COMPOSITION OF FOODS. Water. Proteid. Fat. Carbohydrate. In 100 Parts. Digestible. Cellulose. Ash. 76.7 73.7 36-60 87.7 89.7 13.3 35.6 13.7 42.3 13.1 13.1 10.1 12-15 75.5 87.1 90 73-91 84 20.8 12.6 25-33 3.4 2.0 10.2 7.1 11.5 6.1 7.0 9.9 9.0 23-26 2.0 1.0 2-3 4-8 0.5 1.5 12.1 7-30 3.2 3.1 0.9 0.2 2.1 0.4 0.9 4.6 0.3 l*-2 0.2 0.2 0.5 0.5 0.3 3^7 4.8 5.0 74.8 55.5 69.7 49.2 77.4 68.4 79.0 49-54 20.6 9.3 4-6 3-12 10 0.3 0.3 1.6 0.5 0.6 2.5 0.3 4-7 0.7 1.4 1-2 1-5 4 1.3 EffffS 1.1 3-4 Cows' milk Human milk Wheat flour Wheat bread Rye flour Rye bread Rice 0.7 0.2 0.5 1.1 1.4 1.5 1.0 Corn 1.5 Macaroni Peas, beans, lentils Potatoes 0.5 2-3 1.0 Carrots 0.9 Cabbages Mushrooms Fruit 1.3 1.2 0.5 An examination of this table shows that the animal foods, par- ticularly the meats, are characterized by their small percentage in carbohydrate and by a relatively large amount of proteid or of proteid and fat. With regard to the last two foodstuffs, meats differ * See Konig, " Die menschlichen Nahrungs und Genussmittel " ; and At water and Bryant, " The Chemical Composition of American Food Mate- rials, " Bulletin 28, United States Department of Agriculture, 1899. 656 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. very much among themselves. Some idea of the limits of variation may be obtained from the following table, taken chiefly from Konig's analyses: Water. Proteid. Fat. 73.03 20.96 5.41 72.31 18.88 7.41 75.99 17.11 5.77 72.57 20.05 6.81 62.58 22.32 8.68 10.00 3.00 80.50 71.6 18.8 8.2 Carbohydrate. Ash. Beef, moderately fat . . Veal, fat Mutton, moderately fat, Pork, lean , Ham, salted Pork (bacon), very fat* Mackerel * 0.46 0.07 1.14 1.33 1.33 1.10 6.42 6.5 1.4 The vegetable foods are distinguished, as a rule, by their large percentage in carbohydrates and the relatively small amounts of proteids and fats, as seen, for example, in the composition of rice, corn, wheat, and potatoes. Nevertheless, it will be noticed that the proportion of proteid in some of the vegetables is not at all insignifi- cant. They are characterized by their excess in carbohydrates rather than by a deficiency in proteids. The composition of peas and other leguminous foods is remarkable for the large percentage of proteid, which exceeds that found in meats. Analyses such as are given here are indispensable in determining the true nutritive value of foods. Nevertheless, it must be borne in mind that the chemical composition of a food is not alone sufficient to determine its precise value in nutrition. It is obviously true that it is not what we eat, but what we digest and absorb, that is nutritious to the body; so that, in addition to determining the proportion of food- stuffs in any given food, it is necessary to determine to what extent the several constituents are digested. This factor can be obtained only by actual experiments. It may be said here, however, that in general the proteids of animal foods are more completely digested than are those of vegetables, and with them, therefore, chemical analysis comes nearer to expressing directly the nutritive value. Accessory Articles of Diet. — In addition to the foodstuffs proper our foods contain numerous other substances which in one way or another are useful in nutrition, although not absolutely necessary. These substances, differing in nature and importance, may be classified under the three heads of : Flavors : the various oils or esters that give odor and taste to foods. Condiments: pepper, salt, mustard, etc. Stimulants : alcohol, tea, coffee, cocoa, etc. The specific influence of these substances in digestion and nutrition is considered in the section on nutrition. * Atwater: "The Chemistry of Foods and Nutrition," 1887. COMPOSITION OF FOOD AND ACTION OF ENZYMES. 657 The Chemical Changes of the Foodstuffs during Digestion. — The physiology of digestion consists chiefly in the study of the chemical changes that the food undergoes during its passage through the alimentary canal. It happens that these chemical changes are of a peculiar character. The peculiarity is due to the fact that the changes of digestion are effected through the agency of a group of bodies known as enzymes, or unorganized ferments, whose chemical action is more obscure than that of the ordinary reagents with which we have to deal. It will save repetition to give here certain general facts that are known with reference to these bodies., reserving for later treatment the 'details of the action of the specific enzymes found in the different digestive secretions. ENZYMES AND THEIR ACTION. Historical. — The term fermentation and the idea that it is meant to convey has varied greatly during the course of years. The word at first was applied to certain obvious and apparently spon- taneous changes in organic materials which are accompanied by the liberation of bubbles of gas: such, for instance, as the alcoholic fermentations, in which alcohol is formed from sugar; the acid fer- mentations, as in the souring of milk; and the putrefactive fer- mentations, by means of which animal substances are disintegrated, with the production of offensive odors. These mysterious phenom- ena excited naturally the interest of investigators, and with the development of chemical knowledge numerous other processes were discovered which resemble the typical fermentations in that they seem to be due to specific agents whose mode of action differs from the usual chemical reactions, especially in the fact that the causa- tive agent itself, or the ferment as it is called, is not destroyed or used up in the reaction. Thus it was discovered that germinating barley grains contain a something which can be extracted by water and which can convert starch into sugar (Kirchhoff, 1814). Later this substance was separated by precipitation with alcohol and was given the name of diastase (Payen and Persoz, 1833). Schwann in 1836 demonstrated the existence of a ferment (pepsin) in gastric juice capable of acting upon albuminous substances, and a number of similar bodies were soon discovered: trypsin in the pancreatic juice, amygdalin, invertin, ptyalin, etc. These substances were all designated as ferments, and their action was compared to that of the alcoholic fermentation in yeast, the process of putrefaction, etc. Naturally very many theories have been proposed regarding the cause of the processes of fermentation. For the historical develop- ment and interrelation of these theories references must be made to 42 658 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. special works.* It is sufficient here to say that the brilliant work of Pasteur established the fact that the fermentations in the old sense — alcoholic, acid, and putrefactive — are due to the presence and activity of living organisms. He showed, moreover, that many diseases are likewise due to the activity of minute living organisms, and thus justified the view held by some of the older physicians that there is a close similarity in the processes of fer- mentation and disease. The clear demonstration of the importance of living organisms in some fermentations and the equally clear proof of the existence of another group of ferment actions in which living material is not directly concerned led to a classification which is used even at the present day. This classification divided fer- ments into two great groups : the living or organized ferments, such as the yeast cell, bacteria, etc.; and the non-living or unorganized ferments, such as pepsin, trypsin, etc., which later were generally designated as enzymes (Kuhne). The separation appeared to be entirely satisfactory until Buchner (1897) showed that an unor- ganized ferment, an enzyme (zymase) capable of producing alcohol from sugar, may be extracted from yeast cells. Later the same observer (1903) succeeded in extracting enzymes from the lactic- acid-producing bacteria and the acetic-acid-producing bacteria which are capable of giving the same reactions as the living bacteria. These discoveries indicate clearly that there is no essential difference between the activity of living and non-living ferments. The so- called organized ferments probably produce their effects not by virtue of their specific life-metabolism, but by the manufacture within their substance of specific enzymes. If we can accept this conclusion, then the general explanation of fermentation is to be sought in the nature of the enzymatic processes. Within recent years the study of the enzymes has attracted especial attention. A very large literature is developing, and the subject is therefore correspondingly difficult to present in brief compass. The general point of view regarding the mode of action of enzymes that is most frequently met with to-day is that advocated especially by Ostwald. He assumes, reviving an older view (Berzelius), that the ferment actions are similar to those of catalysis. By catalysis chemists designated a species of reaction which is brought about by the mere contact or presence of certain substances, the catalyzers. Thus, hydrogen and oxygen at ordinary temperatures do not combine to form water, but if spongy platinum is present the two gases unite readily. The platinum does not enter into the reaction, at least it * Consult Green, "The Soluble Ferments and Fermentations," 1899; Effront, " Enzymes and their Applications" (translation by Prescott), 1902; Oppenheimer, "Die Fermente und ihre Wirkungen," second edition, 1903; and Henri, " Lois generates de Taction des diastases, " 1903. COMPOSITION OF FOOD AND ACTION OF ENZYMES. 659 undergoes no change, and it is said, therefore, to act by catalysis. Many similar catalytic reactions are known, and the chemists have reached the important generalization that in such reactions the catalyzer, platinum in the above instance, simply hastens a process which would occur without it, but much more slowly. A catalyzer is a substance, therefore, that alters the velocity of a reaction, but does not initiate it. This idea is illustrated very clearly by the catalysis of hydrogen peroxid. This substance decomposes spontaneously into water and oxygen according to the reaction H202 = H20 + O, but the decomposition is greatly hastened by the presence of a catalyzer. Thus, Bredig has shown that plati- num in very fine suspension, so-called colloidal solution, exerts a marked accelerating influence upon this reaction; one part of the colloidal platinum to 350 million parts of water may still exercise a perceptible effect. Now, the blood and aqueous extracts of various tissues also catalyze the hydrogen peroxid readily, and this effect has been attributed to the action of an enzyme (catalase). The view has been proposed, therefore, that the enzymes of the body act like the catalyzers of inorganic origin : they influence the velocity of certain special reactions. Such a general conception as this unifies the whole subject of fermentation and holds out the hope that the more precise investigations that are possible in the case of the inorganic catalyzers will eventually lead to a better understanding of the underlying physical causes of fermentation. It should be borne in mind, however, that some of the best known of the ferment actions of the body, such as the peptic or tryptic digestion of pro- teid, fit into this view only theoretically and by analogy. As a matter of fact, albumins at ordinary temperatures do not split up spontaneously into the products formed by the action of pepsin; if we consider that the pepsin simply accelerates a reaction already taking place, it must be stated that this reaction at ordinary temperatures is infinitely slow, — that is, practically does not occur. At higher temperatures, however, similar decompositions of al- bumin may be obtained without the presence of an enzyme. Reversible Reactions. — It has been shown that under proper conditions many chemical reactions are reversible, — that is, may take place in opposite directions. For instance, acetic acid and ethyl-alcohol brought together react with the production of ethyl- acetate and water: CH3COOH + C2H5OH = CH3COOC2H5 + H20. Acetic acid. Alcohol. Ethyl-acetate. Water. On the other hand, when ethyl-acetate and water are brought together they react with the formation of some acetic acid and ethyl-alcohol, so that the reaction indicated in the above equation 660 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. takes place in opposite directions, figuratively speaking, — a fact which may be indicated by a symbol of this kind: CH3COOH + C2H5OH ^± CH3COOC2H5 + H20. It is evident that in a reversible reaction of this sort the opposite changes will eventually strike an equilibrium, the solution or mix- ture will contain some of all four substances, and this equilib- rium will remain constant as long as the conditions are unchanged. If the conditions are altered, however, — if, for example, some of the substances formed are removed or the mixture is altered as to its concentration, — then the reaction will proceed unequally in the two directions until a new equilibrium is established. The importance, in the present connection, of this conception of reversibility of reac- tions is found in the fact that a number of the catalytic reactions are also reversible. The catalyzer may not only accelerate a reac- tion between two substances, but may also accelerate the recom- position of the products into the original substances. An excellent instance of this double effect has been obtained by Kastle and Loevenhart in experiments upon one of the enzymes of the animal body, lipase. Lipase is the enzyme which in the body acts upon the neutral fats, converting them into fatty acids and glycerin, — a process that takes place as a usual if not necessary step in the diges- tion and absorption of fats. The authors above named* made use of a simple ester analogous to the fats, ethyl-butyrate, and showed that lipase causes not only an hydrolysis of this substance into ethyl- alcohol and butyric acid, but also a synthesis of the two last-named substances into ethyl-butyrate and water. The reaction effected by the lipase is therefore reversible and may be expressed as : C3H7COOC2H5 + H20 ^± C3H7COOH + C2H5OH. Ethyl-butyrate. Water. Butyric acid. Ethyl-alcohol. Lipase is capable of exerting probably a similar reversible reaction on the fats in the body. Assuming the existence of such an action in the body, it is possible to explain not only the digestion of fats, but also their formation in the tissues and their absorption from the tissues during starvation. That is, according to the conditions of concentration, etc., one and the same enzyme may cause a splitting up of the neutral fat into fatty acids and glycerin or a storing up of neutral fat by the synthesis of fatty acid and glycerin. In the subcutaneous tissues, therefore, fat may be stored, to a certain point, or, if the conditions are altered, the fat that is there may be changed over to the fatty acids and glycerin and be oxidized in the body as food. * Kastle and Loevenhart, "American Chemical Journal," 24, 491, 1900. See also Loevenhart, "American Physiological Journal," 6, 331, 1902. COMPOSITION OF FOOD AND ACTION OF ENZYMES. 661 A similar reversibility has been shown for some of the other enzymes of the body (maltase by Hill, 1898), but whether or not all of them will be shown to possess this power under the conditions of temperature, etc., that prevail in the body can only be determined by actual experiments. The Specificity of Enzymes. — A most interesting feature of the activity of enzymes is that it is specific. The enzymes that act upon the carbohydrates are not capable of affecting the pro- teids or fats, and vice versa. So in the fermentation of closely related bodies such as the double sugars, the enzyme that acts upon the maltose is not capable of affecting the lactose; each re- quires seemingly its own specific enzyme. In fact, there is no clear proof that any single enzyme can produce more than one kind of ferment action. If in any extract or secretion two or more kinds of ferment action can be demonstrated, the tendency at present is to attribute these different activities to the existence of separate and specific enzymes. The pancreatic juice, for example, splits proteids, starches, and fats and curdles milk, and there are assumed to be four different enzymes present, — namely, trypsin, diastase, lipase, and rennin. So if an extract containing diastase is also capable of decomposing hydrogen peroxid it is believed that this latter effect is due to the existence of a special enzyme, catalase. It seems quite probable that this specificity of the different enzymes may be related, as Fischer* has suggested, to the geometrical struc- ture of the, substance acted upon. Each ferment is adapted to act upon or become attached to a molecule with a certain definite structure, — fitted to it, in fact, as a key to its lock. In this respect the action of the so-called hydrolytic enzymes differs markedly from the dilute acids or alkalies which hydrolyze many different substances without indication of any specificity. Attention has been called to the fact that this adaptibility of enzymes to certain specific struc- tures in the molecules acted upon resembles closely the specific activity of the toxins, and many useful and suggestive com- parisons may be drawn between the mode of action of enzymes and toxins. To how complete an extent the idea of the specificity of the different body enzymes may be carried is a matter for future experiments. At present the tendency is to attribute each new kind of activity to a different enzyme, and as a consequence the number of different enzymes supposed to exist in the body is increasing rapidly with the spread of experimental work. Definition and Classification of Enzymes (Ferments). — On the basis of the considerations presented in the preceding paragraphs Oppenheimer suggests the following definition: A ferment is a substance, produced by living cells, which acts by catalysis. The * Fischer, " Zeitschrift f . physiolog. Chemie," 26, 71, 1898. 662 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. ferment itself remains unchanged in this process, and it acts specifi- cally,— that is, each ferment exerts its activity only upon substances whose molecules have a certain definite structural and stereochemical arrangement. With regard to the names and classification of the different enzymes, much difficulty is experienced. There is no consensus among workers as to the system to be followed. Duclaux has sug- gested that an enzyme be designated by the name of the body on which its action is exerted, and that all of them be given the termina- tion ase. The enzyme acting on fat on this system would be named lipase; that on starch, amylase; that on maltose, maltase, etc. The suggestion has been followed in part only, the older enzymes which were first discovered being referred to most frequently under their original names. An entirely satisfactory classification is impossible at present. Having in mind only the needs of animal physi- ology, the following classification will be used in the treatment of the subjects of digestion and nutrition: 1. The proteolytic or proteid-splitting enzymes. Examples: pepsin of gastric juice, trypsin of pancreatic juice. They cause a hydro- lytic cleavage of the proteid molecule. 2. The amylolytic or starch-splitting enzymes. Examples: ptyalin or salivary diastase, amylopsin or pancreatic diastase. Their action is closely similar to that of the classical enzyme of this group — diastase — found in germinating barley grains. They cause a hydrolytic cleavage of the starch molecule. 3. The lipolytic or fat -splitting enzymes. Example: the lipase found in the pancreatic secretion, in the liver, connective tissues, blood, etc. They cause a hydrolytic cleavage of the fat molecule. 4. The sugar-splitting enzymes. These again fall into two subgroups: (a) The inverting enzymes, which convert the double sugars or di- saccharids into the monosaccharids. Examples: maltase, which splits maltose to dextrose; invertase, which splits cane-sugar to dextrose and levulose ; and lactase, which splits milk-sugar (lactose) to dextrose and galactose, (b) The enzymes which split the mono- saccharids. There is evidence of the presence in the tissues of an enzyme capable of splitting the sugar of the blood and tissues (dextrose) into lactic acid. 5. The coagulating enzymes, which convert soluble to insoluble pro- teids. Examples: fibrin ferment (thrombin or thrombase), that causes clotting of the blood, and rennin, that causes clotting of milk. 6. The oxidizing enzymes or oxidases. A group of enzymes which set up oxidation processes. Some of the details of the activity of these enzymes are considered in the discussion of physiological oxidations (p. 833). The enzymes contained in the first, second, third, and fourth (a) of these groups are the ones that play the chief roles in the digestive processes, and it will be noticed that they all act by hydrolysis, — that is, they cause the molecules of the substance to undergo de- composition or cleavage by a reaction with water. Thus, in the conversion of maltose to dextrose by the action of maltase the re- action may be expressed so : COMPOSITION OF FOOD AND ACTION OF ENZYMES. 663 C^H^On + H20 = C6H,206 + C6H1206. Maltose. Dextrose. Dextrose. And the hydrolysis of the neutral fats by lipase may be expressed so: C.H.CCmH.A), + 3H20 = C,H5(OH)8 + SJPuPmOX Tristearin. Glycerin. Stearic acid. General Properties of Enzymes. — The specific reactions of the various enzymes of the body are referred to under separate heads. The following general characteristics may be noted briefly : Solubility. — Most of the enzymes are soluble in water or salt solutions, or in glycerin. By these means they may be extracted conveniently from the various tissues. In some cases, however, such simple methods do not suffice ; the enzyme is destroyed in the process of extraction, and to prove its presence pieces of the tissue or the juice pressed from the tissue must be employed. Temperature. — The body enzymes are characterized by the fact that they are destroyed by high temperatures (60° C. to 80° C.) and that their effect is retarded in part or entirely by low temperatures. Most of them show an optimum activity at temperatures approxi- mating that of the body. Precipitation. — The enzymes are precipitated from their solutions in part at least by excess of alcohol. This precipitation is frequently used in obtaining purified specimens of enzymes. The enzymes, moreover, show an interesting tendency to be carried down mechani- cally by flocculent precipitates produced in their solutions. If proteid present in the solution is precipitated, for instance, the enzymes may be carried down with it in part. Incompleteness of their Action. — In any given mixture of a substance and its enzyme the action of the latter is not complete, — that is, all of the substance does not disappear. An explanation for this fact has been found in the reversibility of the action of the enzyme. If the reaction proceeds in both directions, then evidently under fixed conditions a final equilibrium will be reached in which no further apparent change takes place, although in reality the condition is not one of rest, but of balance between opposing proc- esses proceeding at a definite rate. Within the body itself, on the contrary, the action of an enzyme may be complete, since the products are removed by absorption and the possibility of a re- versed reaction is removed. In some cases — for instance, the co- agulating enzymes — the action is apparently always complete. Active and Inactive Form. — In many cases it can be shown that the enzyme exists within the cell producing it in an inactive form or even when secreted it may still be inactive. This antecedent or inactive stage is usually designated as zymogen. The zymogen may be stored in the cell in the form of granules which are converted into 664 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. active enzyme at the moment of secretion, or it may be secreted in inactive form and require the co-operation of some other substance before it is capable of effecting its normal reaction. In such cases the second substance is said to activate the enzyme. An example is found in the case of the trypsin of the pancreatic secretion. PARTIAL LIST OF THE ENZYMES CONCERNED IN THE PROC- ESSES OF DIGESTION AND NUTRITION. f i -Sail ^■2 J Enzyme. Ptyalin (sali- vary diastase. A m y lop s i n (pancreatic diastase) . Liver diastase. Invert ase. Maltase. Lactase. Glycolytic? Lipase (steap- sin). 'S '55 2 Pepsin. Trypsin. Erepsin. Group of auto- . lytic enzymes. Guanase. Adenase. Oxidases. Catalase. Where Chiefly Found. Salivary secretion. Pancreatic secre- tion. Liver. Small intestine. Small intestine, sal- ivary and pan- creatic secre- tion, liver. Small intestine. Muscles? Pancreatic secre- tion, fat tissues, blood, etc. Gastric juice. Pancreatic juice. Small intestine. Tissues generally. Thymus, adrenals, pancreas. Spleen, pancreas, liver, Lungs, liver. Many tissues. Action. Converts starch to sugar (maltose) . Converts starch to sugar (maltose) . Converts glycogen to dex- trose (maltose). Converts cane-sugar to dextrose and levulose. Converts maltose to dex- trose. Converts lactose to dex- trose and galactose. Splits and oxidizes dex- trose. Splits neutral fats to fatty acids and glycerin. Converts proteids to pep- tones and proteoses. Splits proteids into sim- pler crystalline prod- ucts. Splits peptones into sim- pler products. Split proteids into nitrog- enous bases and amido- bodies. Converts guanin to xan- thin. Converts adenin to hypo- xanthin. Cause oxidation of organic substances. Decomposes hydrogen peroxid. Chemical Composition of the Enzymes. — It was formerly believed that the enzymes belong to the group of proteids. They are formed from living matter, and the solutions as usually prepared give proteid reactions. Increased study , however, has made this belief uncertain. The enzymes cling to the proteids when precipitated, and it seems possible that the proteid reactions of their solutions may COMPOSITION OF FOOD AND ACTION OF ENZYMES. 665 be due, therefore, to" an incomplete purification. In fact, it is stated that solutions of some of the enzymes may be prepared (pepsin, invertase, thrombin) which show ferment activity, but give no proteid reactions. Much of the older work upon the composition of supposedly purified preparations of enzymes is not accepted to-day, on the ground that the evidence for the purity of the preparations is insufficient. In spite, however, of the very great amount of attention" that has been paid to these substances in recent years, there is at present no agreement as to their chemical structure. Some au- thorities (Arthus) have gone so far as to suggest that the enzymes, or more properly enzyme actions, are not due to definite material substances, but are to be classified as forms of energy like heat, electricity, etc. The suggestion is not very helpful, but it indicates forcibly the present uncertainty regarding the real nature of these bodies. CHAPTER XLI. THE SALIVARY GLANDS AND THEIR DIGESTIVE ACTION. The first of the secretions with which the food comes into contact is the saliva. This is a mixed secretion from the large salivary glands and the small unnamed mucous and serous glands that open into the mouth cavity. The Salivary Glands. — The salivary glands in man are three in number on each side — the parotid, the submaxillary, and the sublingual. The parotid gland communicates with the mouth by a large duct (Stenson's duct) which opens upon the inner surface of the cheek opposite the second molar tooth of the upper jaw. The submaxillary gland lies below the lower jaw, and its duct (Wharton's duct) opens into the mouth cavity at the side of the frenum of the tongue. The sublingual gland lies in the floor of the mouth to the side of the frenum and opens into the mouth cavity by a number (eight to twenty) of small ducts, known as the ducts of Rivinus. One larger duct that runs parallel with the duct of Whar- ton and opens separately into the mouth cavity is sometimes present in man. It is known as the duct of Bartholin and occurs normally in the dog. The course of the nerve fibers supplying the large salivary glands is interesting in view of the physiological results of their stimulation. The description here given applies especially to their arrangement in the dog. These glands receive their nerve supply from two general sources, — namely, the bulbar autonomics (or cerebral fibers) and the sympathetic autonomics. The parotid gland receives its bulbar autonomic fibers from the glossopharyngeal or ninth cranial nerve; they pass into a branch of this nerve known as the tympanic branch or nerve of Jacobson, thence to the small superficial pe- trosal nerve, through which they reach the otic ganglion. From this ganglion they pass (postganglionic fibers) by way of the auricu- lotemporal branch of the inferior maxillary division of the fifth cranial nerve to the parotid gland (Fig. 255). The sympathetic autonomics pass to the superior cervical ganglion by way of the cervical sympathetic (Fig. 105) and thence as postganglionic fibers in branches which accompany the arteries distributed to the gland. The bulbar autonomic supply for the submaxillary and sublingual 666 THE SALIVARY GLANDS. 667 glands arises from the brain in the facial nerve and passes out in the chorda tympani branch (Fig. 256). This latter nerve, after emerging from the tympanic cavity through the Glaserian fissure, joins the Tdroui , Ganglion. Fig. 255. — Schematic representation of the course of the cerebral fibers to the parotid gland. lingual nerve. After running with this nerve for a short distance, the secretory (and vasodilator) nerve fibers- destined for the sub- maxillary and sublingual glands branch off and pass to the glands, ^Branches to. __ Sub- ■MtaxilLa.ry- am Su.6Un.yua.L branches Sublinyual^&jTongiLe Gatwliotu Fig. 256. -Schematic representation of the course of the chorda tympani nerve to the submaxillary gland. following the course of the ducts. Where the chorda tympani fibers leave the lingual there is a small ganglion which has received the name of submaxillary ganglion. The nerve fibers to the glands 668 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. pass close to this ganglion, but Langley has shown that only those destined for the sublingual gland really connect with the nerve cells of the ganglion, and he suggests, therefore, that it should be called the sublingual instead of the submaxillary ganglion. The nerve fibers for the submaxillary gland make connections with nerve cells lying mainly within the hilus of the gland itself. The supply of sympathetic autonomics has the same general course as those for the parotid, — namely, through the cervical sympathetic to the superior cervical ganglion and thence to the glands. Histological Structure. — The salivary glands belong to the type of compound tubular glands. That is, the secreting portions are tubular in shape, although in cross-sections these tubes may pre- sent various outlines according as the plane of the section passes through them. The parotid is described usually as a typical serous or albuminous gland. Its secreting epithelium is composed of cells which in the fresh condition as well as in preserved specimens contain numerous fine granules and its secretion contains some albumin. The submaxillary gland differs in histology in different animals. In some, as the dog or cat, the secretory tubes are composed chiefly or exclusively of epithelial cells of the mucous type. In man the gland is of a mixed type, the secretory tubes containing both mucous and albuminous cells. The sublingual gland in man also contains both varieties of cells, although the mucous cells predominate. In accordance with these histological characteristics it is found that the secretion from the submaxillary and sublingual glands is thick and mucilaginous as compared with that from the parotid. In the mucous glands another variety of cell, the so-called demilunes or crescent cells, is frequently met with, and the physiological significance of these cells has been the subject of much discussion. The demilunes are crescent-shaped, granular cells lying between the mucous cells and the base- ment membrane, and not in contact, therefore, with the central lumen of the tube. According to Heidenhain, these demilunes are for the purpose of replacing the mucous cells. In consequence of long-continued activity the mucous cells may disintegrate and disappear, and the demilunes then develop into new mucous cells. Another view is that the demilunes represent distinct secretory cells of the albuminous type, while others assert that they are a specific type of cell with probably specific functions.* The salivary glands possess definite secretory nerves which when stimulated cause the formation of a secretion. This fact indicates that there must be a direct contact of some kind between the gland cells and the terminations of the secretory fibers. The ending of the nerve fibers in the submaxillary and sublingual glands has been de- scribed by a number of observers.! The accounts differ somewhat as to details of the finer anatomy, but it seems to be clearly established that the secretory fibers from the chorda tympani end first around the * See Noll, " Archiv f . Physiologie, " 1902, suppl. volume, 166. f See Huber, " Journal of Experimental Medicine," 1, 281, 1896. THE SALIVARY GLANDS. ■ 669 intrinsic nerve ganglion cells of the glands (preganglionic fibers), and from these latter cells axons (postganglionic fibers) are distributed to the secreting cells, passing to these cells along the ducts. The nerve fibers terminate in a plexus upon the membrana propria of the alveoli, and from this plexus fine fibrils pass inward to end on and between the secreting cells. It would seem from these observations that the nerve fibrils do not penetrate or fuse with the gland cells, as was formerly supposed, but form a terminal network in contact with the cells, following thus the general schema for the connection between nerve fibers and peripheral tissues. Composition of the Secretion. — The saliva as it is found in the mouth is a colorless or opalescent, turbid, and mucilaginous liquid of weakly alkaline reaction and a specific gravity of about 1.003. It may contain numerous flat cells derived from the epithelium of the mouth, and the peculiar spherical cells known as salivary corpuscles, which seem to be altered leucocytes. The important constituents of- the secretion are mucin, a diastatic enzyme known as ptyalin, maltase, traces of proteid and of potassium sulphocyanid, and inorganic salts such as potassium and sodium chlorid, potassium sulphate, sodium carbonate, and calcium carbonate and phosphate. The carbonates are particularly abundant in the saliva, and the secretion in addition contains much carbon dioxid in solution. Thus, Pfliiger found that 65 volumes per cent, of C02 might be obtained from the saliva, of which 42.5 per cent, was in the form of carbon- ates. The amount of C02 in solution and combined is an indication of the active chemical changes in the gland. Of the organic constituents of the saliva the proteid exists in small and variable quantities, and its exact nature is not determined. The mucin gives to the saliva its ropy, mucilaginous character. This substance belongs to the group of combined proteids, glyco- proteids (see appendix), consisting of a proteid combined with a carbohydrate group. The most interesting constituent of the mixed saliva is the ptyalin or salivary diastase. This body belongs to the group of enzymes or unorganized ferments, whose general and specific properties have been described. In some animals (dog) ptyalin seems to be normally absent from the fresh saliva. The secretions of the parotid and the submaxillary glands can be obtained separately by inserting a cannula into the openings of the ducts in the mouth, or, according to the method of Pawlow, by trans- ferring the end of the duct so that it opens upon the skin instead of in the mouth, making thus a salivary fistula. The secretion of the sublingual can only be obtained in sufficient quantities for analysis from the lower animals. Examination of the separate secretions shows that the main difference lies in the fact that the parotid saliva contains no mucin, while that of the submaxillary and especially of 670 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. the sublingual gland is rich in mucin. The parotid saliva of man seems to be particularly rich in ptyalin as compared with that of the submaxillary, while the secretion of the latter and that of the sub- lingual gland give a stronger alkaline reaction than the parotid saliva. The Secretory Nerves. — The existence of secretory nerves to the salivary glands was discovered by Ludwig in 1851. The discovery is particularly interesting in that it marks the beginning of our knowl- edge of this kind of nerve fiber. Ludwig found that stimulation of the chorda tympani nerve causes a flow of saliva from the submaxil- lary gland. He established also several important facts with regard to the pressure and composition of the secretion which will be referred to presently. It was afterward shown that the salivary glands receive a double nerve supply, — in part by way of the cervical sympathetic and in part through cerebral nerves. It was discovered also that not only are secretory fibers carried to the glands by these paths, but that vasomotor fibers are contained in the same nerves, and the arrangement of these latter fibers is such that the cerebral nerves contain vasodilator fibers that cause a dilatation of the small arteries in the glands and an accelerated blood-flow, while the sym- pathetic carries vasoconstrictor fibers whose stimulation causes a constriction of the small arteries and a diminished blood-flow. The effect of stimulating these two sets of fibers is found to vary somewhat in different animals. For purposes of description we may confine ourselves to the effects observed on clogs, since much of our funda- mental knowledge upon the subject is derived from Heidenhain's* experiments upon this animal. If the chorda tympani nerve is stimulated by weak induction shocks, the gland begins to secrete promptly, and the secretion, by proper regulation of the stimulation, may be kept up for hours. The secretion thus obtained is thin and watery, flows freely, is abundant in amount, and contains not more than 1 or 2 per cent, of total solids. At the same time there is an increased flow of blood through the gland. The whole gland takes on a redder hue, the veins are distended, and if cut the blood that flows from them is of a redder color than in the resting gland, and may show a distinct pulse — all of which points to a dilatation of the small arteries. If now the sympathetic fibers are stimulated, quite different results are obtained. The secretion is relatively small in amount, flows slowly, is thick and turbid, and may contain as much as 6 per cent, of total solids. At the same time the gland becomes pale, and if the veins be cut the flow from them is slower than in the resting gland, thus indicating that a vasoconstriction has occurred. The increased vascular supply to the gland accompanying the *"Pfliiger's Archiv fur die gesammte Physiologie, " 17, 1, 1878; also in Hermann's "Handbuch der Physiologie/' 1883, vol. v, part i. THE SALIVARY GLANDS. 671 abundant flow of "chorda saliva" and the diminished flow of blood during the scanty secretion of " sympathetic saliva " suggest naturally the idea that the whole process of secretion may be, at bottom, a vasomotor phenomenon, the amount of secretion depending only on the quantity and pressure of the blood flowing through the gland. It has been shown conclusively that this idea is erroneous and that definite secretory fibers exist. The following facts may be quoted in support of this statement: (1) Ludwig showed that if a mercury manometer is connected with the duct of the submaxillary gland and the chorda is then stimulated for a certain time, the pressure in the duct may become greater than the blood-pressure in the gland. This fact shows that the secretion is not derived entirely by processes of filtration from the blood. (2) If the blood-flow be shut off completely from the gland, stimulation of the chorda still gives a secretion for a short time. (3) If atropin is injected into the gland, stimulation of the chorda causes vascular dilatation, but no secretion. This may be explained by supposing that the atropin paralyzes the secretory, but not the dilator, fibers. (4) Hydro- chlorate of quinin injected into the gland causes vascular dilatation, but no secretion. In this case the secretory fibers are still irritable, since stimulation of the chorda gives the usual secretion. A still more marked difference between the effect of stimulation of the cerebral and the sympathetic fibers may be observed in the case of the parotid gland in the dog. Stimulation of the cerebral fibers alone in any part of their course gives an abundant, thin, and watery saliva, poor in solid constituents. Stimulation of the sympathetic fibers alone (provided the cerebral fibers have not been stim- ulated shortly before and the tympanic nerve has been cut to prevent a reflex effect) gives usually no perceptible secretion at all. But in this last stimulation a marked effect is produced upon the gland, in spite of the absence of a visible secretion. This is shown by the fact that subsequent or simultaneous stimulation of the cerebral fibers causes a secretion very unlike that given by the cerebral fibers alone, in that it is very rich indeed in organic constituents. The amount of organic matter in the secretion may be tenfold that of the saliva obtained by stimulation of the cerebral fibers alone. Relation of the Composition of the Secretion to the Strength of Stimu- lation.— If the stimulus to the chorda is gradually increased in strength, care being taken not to fatigue the gland, the chemical composition of the secretion is found to change with regard to the relative amounts of the water, the salts, and the organic material. The water and the salts increase in amount with the increased strength of stimulus up to a certain maximal limit, which for the salts is about 0.77 per cent. It is important to observe that this effect may be obtained from a perfectly fresh gland as well as from a 672 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. gland which had previously been secreting actively. With regard to the organic constituents the precise result obtained depends on the condition of the gland. If previous to the stimulation the gland was in a resting condition and unfatigued, then increased strength of stimulation is followed at first by a rise in the percentage of organic constituents, and this rise in the beginning is more marked than in the case of the salts. But with continued stimulation the increase in organic material soon ceases, and finally the amount begins actually to diminish, and may fall to a low point in spite of the stronger stimulation. On the other hand, if the gland at the beginning of the experiment had been previously worked to a considerable extent, then an increase in the stimulating current, while it augments the amount of water and salts, either may have no effect at all upon the organic constituents or may cause only a temporary increase, quickly followed by a fall. Similar results may be obtained from stimulation of the cerebral nerves of the parotid gland. The ab.ove facts led Heidenhain to believe that the conditions determining the secretion of the organic material are different from those controlling the water and salts, and he gave a rational explanation of the differences observed, in his theory of trophic and secretory fibers. Theory of Trophic and Secretory Nerve Fibers. — This theory supposes that two physiological varieties of nerve fibers are distrib- uted to the salivary glands. One of these varieties controls the secretion of the water and inorganic salts and its fibers may be called secretory fibers proper, while the other, to which the name trophic is given, causes the formation of the organic constituents of the secre- tion, probably by a direct influence on the metabolism of the cells. Were the trophic fibers to act alone, the organic products would be formed within the cell, but there would be no visible secretion, and this is the hypothesis which Heidenhain uses to explain the results of the experiment described above upon stimulation of the sympathetic fibers to the parotid of the dog. In this animal, apparently, the sympathetic branches to the parotid contain exclusively or almost exclusively trophic fibers, while in the cerebral branches both trophic and secretory fibers proper are present. The results of stimulation of the cerebral and sympathetic branches to the submaxillary gland of the same animal may be explained in terms of this theory by supposing that in the latter nerve trophic fibers preponderate, and in the former the secretory fibers proper. It is obvious that this anatomical separation of the two sets of fibers along the cerebral and sympathetic paths may be open to individual variations, and that dogs may be found in which the sym- pathetic branches to the parotid glands contain secretory fibers proper, and therefore give some flow of secretion on stimulation. These variations might also be expected to be more marked when THE SALIVARY GLANDS. 673 animals of different groups are compared. Thus, Langley* finds that in cats the sympathetic saliva from the submaxillary gland is less viscid than the chorda saliva, — just the reverse of what occurs in the dog. To apply Heidenhain's theory to this case it is necessary to assume that in the cat the trophic fibers run chiefly in the chorda. An interesting fact with reference to the secretion of the parotid in dogs has been noted by Langley, and is of special interest, since, although it may be reconciled with the theory of trophic and secretory fibers, it is at the same time suggestive of an incompleteness in this theory. As has been said, stimu- lation of the sympathetic in the dog causes usually no secretion from the pa- rotid. Langleyf finds, however, that, if the tympanic nerve is stimulated just previously, stimulation of the sympathetic causes an abundant, but brief, now from the parotid. One may explain this in terms of the theory by as- suming that the sympathetic does contain a few secretory fibers proper, but that ordinarily their action is too feeble to start the flow of water. Pre- vious stimulation of the tympanic nerve, however, leaves the gland cells in a more irritable condition, so that the few secretory fibers proper in the sympathetic branches are now effective in producing a flow of water. The way in which the trophic fibers act has been briefly indicated. They may be supposed to set up metabolic changes in the protoplasm of the cells, leading to the formatton of certain definite products, such as mucin or ptyalin. That such changes do occur is abundantly shown by microscopical examination of the resting and the active gland, the details of which will be given presently. In general, these changes may be supposed to be catabolic in nature ; that is, to consist in a disassociation or breaking down of the complex living material, with the formation of the simpler and more stable organic con- stituents of the secretion. There is evidence to show that these gland cells during activity form fresh material from the nourishment supplied by the blood ; that is, that anabolic or building-up processes occur along with the catabolic changes. The latter are the more obvious, and are the changes which are usually associated with the action of the trophic nerve fibers. It is possible, also, that the ana- bolic or growth changes may be under the control of separate fibers, for which the name anabolic fibers would be appropriate. Satis- factory proof of the existence of a separate set of anabolic fibers has not yet been furnished. The method of action of the secretory fibers proper is difficult to understand. At present the theories suggested are entirely specula- tive. Heidenhain's own view rests upon the fact that no more water leaves the blood capillaries than afterwards appears in the secretion; that is, no matter how long the secretion continues, the gland does not become edematous nor does the velocity of the lymph-stream in the lymphatics of the gland increase. This being the case, we must suppose that the stream of water is regulated by the secretion, that is, by the activity of the gland-cells. If we suppose that some con- * "Journal of Physiology," 1. 96, 1878. f Ibid., 10, 291, 1889. 43 674 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. stituent of these cells has an attraction for water, or, to use the modern expression, exerts a high osmotic pressure, then, while the gland is in the resting state, water will diffuse first from the base- ment membrane, this in turn supplies its loss from the surrounding lymph, and the lymph obtains the same amount of water from the blood. As the amount of water in the cell increases a point is reached at which an equilibrium is established, and the osmotic stream from blood to cells comes to a standstill. The water in the cells does not escape into the lumen of the tubule or of the secretion capillaries, because the periphery of the cell is modified to form a layer offering considerable resistance to filtration. The action of the secretory fibers proper consists in so altering the structure of this limiting layer of the cells that it offers less resistance to filtration; consequently the water under tension in the cells escapes into the lumen, and the osmotic pressure of its substance again starts a stream of water from capillaries to cells, which continues as long as the nerve-stimu- lation is effective. Histological Changes during Activity. — The cells of both the albuminous and mucous glands undergo distinct histological changes in consequence of prolonged activity, and these changes may be recognized both in preparations from the fresh gland and in preserved specimens. In the parotid gland Heidenhain studied the changes in stained sections after hardening in alcohol. In the resting gland the cells are compactly filled with granules that stain readily and are imbedded in a clear ground substance that does not stain. The nucleus is small and more or less irregular in outline. After stimula- tion of the tympanic nerve the cells show but little alteration, but stimulation of the sympathetic produces a marked change. The cells become smaller, the nuclei more rounded, and the granules more closely packed. This last appearance seems, however, to be due to the hardening reagents used. A truer picture of what occurs may be obtained from a study of sections of the fresh gland. Lang- ley,* who first used this method, describes his results as follows: When the animal is in a fasting condition the cells have a granular appearance throughout their substance, the outlines of the different cells being faintly marked by light lines (Fig. 257, A). When the gland is made to secrete by giving the animal food, by injecting pilocarpin, or by stimulating the sympathetic nerves, the granules begin to disappear from the outer borders of the cells (Fig. 257, B), so that each cell now shows an outer, clear border and an inner, granular one. If the stimulation is continued the granules become fewer in number and are collected near the lumen and the margins of the cells, the clear zone increases in extent, and the cells become smaller (Fig. 257, C, D). Evidently the granular material is used * "Journal of Physiology," 2, 260, 1879. THE SALIVARY GLANDS. 675 in some way to make the organic material of the secretion. Since the ptyalin is a conspicuous organic constituent of the secretion, it is assumed that the granules in the resting gland contain the ptyalin, or rather the preliminary material from which the ptyalin is con- structed during the act of secretion. On this latter assumption the granules are frequently spoken of as zymogen granules. During the act of secretion two distinct processes seem to be going on in the cell, leaving out of consideration, for the moment, the secretion of the water and the salts. In the first place, the zymogen granules undergo a change such that they are forced or dissolved out of the cell, and, second, a constructive metabolism or anabolism is set up, leading to D Fig. 257. — Parotid gland of the rabbit in a fresh state, showing portions of the secret- ing tubules: A, In a resting condition; B, after secretion caused by pilocarpin; C, after stronger secretion, pilocarpin and stimulation of sympathetic; D, after long-continued stimulation of sympathetic. — (After Langley.) the formation of new protoplasmic material from the substances contained in the blood and lymph. The new material thus formed is the clear, non-granular substance, which appears first toward the basal sides of the cells. We may suppose that the clear substance during the resting periods undergoes metabolic changes, whether of a catabolic or anabolic character can not be safely asserted, leading to the formation of new granules, and the cells are again ready to form a secretion of normal composition. It should be borne in mind that in these experiments the glands were stimulated beyond normal limits. Under ordinary conditions" the cells are probably never depleted of their granular material to the extent represented in the figures. 676 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. Fig. 258. — Mucous gland: submaxillary of dog; rest- ing stage. In the cells of the mucous glands changes equally marked may be observed after prolonged activity. In stained sections of the resting gland the cells are large and clear (Fig. 258), with flattened nuclei placed well toward the base of the cell. When the gland is made to secrete the nuclei become more spherical and lie more toward the middle of the cell, and the cells themselves be- come distinctly smaller. After prolonged secretion the changes become more marked (Fig. 259) and, according to Heidenhain, some of the mucous cells may break down comr pletely. According to most of the later observ- ers, however, the mucous cells do not actually dis- integrate, but form again new material during the period of rest, as in the case of the goblet cells of the intestine. In the mucous as in the albuminous cells observations upon pieces of the fresh gland seem to give more reliable results than those upon preserved specimens. Langley* has shown that in the fresh mucous cells of the submax- illary gland numerous large granules may be discovered, about 125 to 250 to a cell. These granules are comparable to those found in the goblet cells, and may be inter- preted as consisting of mu- cin or some preparatory material from which mucin is formed. The granules are sensitive to reagents; addition of water causes them to swell up and dis- appear. It may be as- sumed that this happens during secretion, the gran- ules becoming converted to a mucin mass which is extruded from the cell. Action of Atropin, Pilocarpin, and Nicotin upon the Secre- tory Nerves. — The action of drugs upon the salivary glands and ♦"Journal of Physiology," 10, 433, 1889. Fig. 259. — Mucous gland: submaxillary of dog after eight hours' stimulation of the chorda tympani. THE SALIVARY GLANDS. 677 their secretions belongs properly to pharmacology, but the effects of the three drugs mentioned are so decided that they have a peculiar physiological interest. Atropin in small doses injected either into the blood or into the gland duct prevents the action of the cerebral autonomic fibers (tympanic nerve or chorda tympani) upon the glands. This effect may be explained by assuming that the atropin paralyzes the endings of the cerebral fibers in the glands. That it does not act directly upon the gland cells themselves seems to be assured by the interesting fact that, with doses sufficient to throw out entirely the secreting action of the cerebral fibers, the sympathetic fibers are still effective when stimulated. Pilocarpin has directly the opposite effect to atropin. In minimal doses it sets up a continuous secretion of saliva, which may be explained upon the supposition that it stimulates the endings of the secretory fibers in the gland. Within certain limits these drugs antagonize each other, — that is, the effect of pilocarpin may be removed by the sub- sequent application of atropin, and vice versa, Nicotin, according to the experiments of Langley,* prevents the action of the secretory nerves, not by affecting the gland cells or the endings of the nerve fibers around them, but by paralyzing the connections between the nerve fibers and the gang lion cells through which the fibers pass on their way to the gland, — that is, the connection between the pre- ganglionic and postganglionic fibers. If, for example, the superior cervical ganglion is painted with a solution of nicotin, stimulation of the cervical sympathetic below the gland gives no secretion; stim- ulation, however, of the fibers in the ganglion or between the ganglion and gland gives the usual effect. By the use of this drug Langley is led to believe that the cells of the so-called submaxillary ganglion are really intercalated in the course of the fibers to the sublingual gland, while the nerve cells with which the submaxillary fibers make connection are found chiefly in the hilus of the gland itself. Paralytic Secretion. — A remarkable phenomenon in connection with the salivary glands is the so-called paralytic secretion. It has been known for a long time that if the chorda tympani is cut the submaxillary gland after a certain time, one to three days, begins to secrete slowly, and the secretion continues uninterruptedly for a long period — as long, perhaps, as several weeks — and eventually the gland itself undergoes atrophy. Langley states that section of the chorda on one side is followed by a continuous secretion from the glands on both sides; the secretion from the gland of the opposite side he designates as the antiparalytic or antilytic secretion. After section of the chorda the nerve fibers peripheral to the section degenerate,, the process being completed within a few days. These fibers, how- ever, do not run directly to the gland cell; they terminate in end * " Proceedings of the Royal Society, " London, 46, 423, 1889. 678 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. arborizations around sympathetic nerve cells placed somewhere along their course, — in the sublingual ganglion, for instance, or within the gland substance itself. It is the axons from these second nerve units that end around the secreting cells. Langley has accumulated some facts to show that within the period of continuance of the paralytic secretion (five to six weeks) the fibers of the sympathetic cells are still irritable to stimulation. He is inclined to believe, therefore, that the continuous secretion is due to a continuous excitation, from some cause, of the local nervous mechanism in the gland. On the histo- logical side it is stated * that after section of the chorda the resulting degenerative changes affect only the cytoplasm, while after the section of the sympathetic the nuclei of the cells are affected, and indeed to some extent on the sound as well as on the injured side. Normal Mechanism of Salivary Secretion. — Under normal conditions the flow of saliva from the salivary glands is the result of a reflex stimulation of the secretory nerves. The sensory fibers concerned in this reflex must be chiefly fibers of the glossopharyngeal and lingual nerves supplying the mouth and tongue. Sapid bodies and various other chemical or mechanical stimuli applied to the tongue or mucous membrane of the mouth produce a flow of saliva. The normal flow during mastication must be effected by a reflex of this kind, the sensory impulse being carried to a center and thence transmitted through the efferent nerves to the glands. It is found that section of the chorda prevents the reflex, in spite of the fact that the sympathetic fibers are still intact. No satisfactory explanation of the normal functions of the secretory fibers in the sympathetic has yet been given. Various authors have suggested that possibly the three large salivary glands respond normally to different stimuli. This view has lately been supported by Pawlow, who reports that in the dog at least the parotid and the submaxillary may react quite differently. When fistulas were made of the ducts of these glands it was found that the submaxillary responded readily to a great number of stimuli, such as the sight of food, chewing of meats, acids, etc. The parotid, on the contrary, seemed to react only when dry food, dry powdered meat, or bread was placed in the mouth. Dryness in this case appeared to be the efficient stimulus. Pawlow lays great stress upon the adaptability of the secretion of saliva to the character of the material chewed. Dry, solid food stimulates a large flow of saliva, such as is necessary in order to chew it properly and to form it into a bolus for swallowing. Foods containing much water, on the contrary, excite but little flow of saliva. If one places a handful of clean stones in the mouth of a dog he will move them around with his tongue for a while and then drop them from his mouth; but little or no saliva is secreted. If the same material is given in the form of fine sand a rich flow of saliva is produced, and the necessity for the reflex is evident in this case, since otherwise the material could not be conveniently removed from the mouth. ♦Gerhardt, "Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 97, 317, 1903. THE SALIVARY GLANDS. 679 Such adaptations must be regarded from the physiological point of view as special reflexes depending upon some difference in the nervous mechanism set into play.* Since the flow of saliva is normally a definite reflex, we should expect a distinct salivary secretion center. This center has been located by physiological means in the medulla oblongata; its exact position is not clearly defined, but possibly it is represented by the nuclei of origin of the secretory fibers which leave the medulla by way of the facial and glossopharyngeal nerves. Owing to the wide connections of nerve cells in the central nervous system, we should expect this center to be affected by stimuli from various sources. As a matter of fact, it is known that the center and through it the glands may be called into activity by stimulation of the sensory fibers of the sciatic, splanchnic, and particularly the vagus nerves. So, too, various psychical acts, such as the thought of savory food and the feeling of nausea preceding vomiting, may be accompanied by a flow of saliva, the effect in this case being due probably to stimula- tion of the secretion center by nervous impulses descending from the higher nerve centers. Lastly, the medullary center may be inhibited as well as stimulated. The well-known effect of fear, embarrassment, or anxiety in producing a parched throat may be explained in this way as due to the inhibitory action of nerve impulses arising in the cerebral centers. Electrical Changes in the Gland during Activity. — It has been shown that the salivary as well as other glands suffer certain changes in electrical potential during activity which are comparable in a gen- eral way to the "action currents" observed in muscles and nerves. f The Digestive Action of Saliva — Ptyalin. — The digestive action proper of the saliva is limited to the starchy food. In human beings and most mammals the saliva contains an active enzyme belonging to the group of diastases and designated usually as ptyalin or salivary diastase. It may be prepared in purified form from saliva by precipitation with alcohol, but its chemical nature, like that of the other enzymes, is still an unsolved problem. Saliva or preparations of ptyalin act readily upon boiled starch, converting it into sugar and dextrin. This action may be demonstrated very readily by holding a little starch paste or starchy food, such as boiled potatoes, in the mouth for a few moments. If the solution is then examined the presence of sugar is readily shown by its reducing action on solutions of copper sulphate (Fehling's solution). There is no doubt that the action of ptyalin upon the starch is hydrolytic. Under the influence * See Pawlow, " The Work of the Digestive Glands, " translation by Thompson, London, 1902; also " Ergebnisse der Physiologie, " vol iii, part i, 1904, and "Archives internationales de physiologie," 1, 119, 1904. f See Biedermann, " Electro-physiology, " translation by Welby, London, 1896. 680 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. of the enzyme the starch molecules take up water and undergo cleavage into simpler molecules. The steps in the process and the final products have been investigated by a very large number of workers, but much yet remains in doubt. The following points seem to be determined: The end-result of the reaction is the formation of maltose, a disaccharid, having the general formula C12H22On, and some form of dextrin, a non-crystallizable poly- saccharid. When the digestion is effected in a vessel some dextrose (C6H1206) may be found among the products, but this is explained on the assumption that there is present in the saliva some maltase, an enzyme capable of splitting maltose into dextrose. So far as the ptyalin itself is concerned, its specific action is to convert starch to maltose and dextrin. It seems very certain, however, that a number of intermediate products are formed consisting of a variety of dex- trins, so that the hydrolysis probably takes place in successive stages. There is little agreement as to the exact nature of the in- termediate dextrins. The following facts, however, may be easily demonstrated in a salivary digestion carried on in a vessel and ex- amined from time to time. The starch at first gives its deep-blue reaction with iodin; later, instead of a blue, a red reaction is obtained with iodin, and this has been attributed to a special form of dextrin, erythrodextrin, so named on account of its red reaction. Still later this reaction fails and chemical examination shows the presence of maltose and a form of dextrin which gives no color reaction with iodin and is therefore named achroodextrin. While the number of intermediate products may be large, the main result of the action of the ptyalin is expressed by the following simple schema : The products formed in this reaction are probably not absorbed as such. The absorption takes place mainly no doubt after the food reaches the small intestine, and we have evidence, as will be stated, that before absorption the maltose and the dextrin are acted upon by the inverting enzymes (maltase) and converted into the simple sugar, dextrose. The ptyalin digestion seems therefore to be pre- paratory, and the combined action of ptyalin and maltase is necessary to get the starch into a condition ready for nutrition. By way of comparison it is interesting to remember that when starch is boiled with dilute acids it is hydrolyzed at once to dextrose. A question of practical importance is as to how far salivary digestion affects the starchy foods under usual circumstances. The chewing process in the mouth thoroughly mixes the food and saliva, or should do so, but the bolus is swallowed much too quickly to enable the enzyme to complete its action. In the stomach the gastric juice is sufficiently THE SALIVARY GLANDS. 681 acid to destroy the ptyalin, and it was therefore supposed formerly that salivary digestion is promptly arrested on the entrance of the food into the stomach, and is therefore normally of but little value as a digestive process. Our recent increase in knowledge regarding the conditions in the stomach (p. 642) shows, on the contrary, that some of the food in an ordinary meal may remain in the fundic end of the stomach for an hour or more untouched by the acid secretion. There is every reason to believe, therefore, that salivary digestion may be carried on in the stomach to an important extent. Conditions Influencing the Action of Ptyalin. — Temperature. — As in the case of the other enzymes, ptyalin is very susceptible to changes of temperature. At 0° C. its activity is said to be suspended entirely. The intensity of its action increases with increase of temperature from this point, and reaches its maximum at about 40° C. If the temperature is raised much beyond this point, the action decreases, and at from 65° to 70° C. the enzyme is destroyed. In these latter points ptyalin differs from diastase, the enzyme of malt. Diastase shows a maximum action at 50° C. and is destroyed at 80° C. Effect of Reaction. — The normal reaction of saliva is slightly alkaline. Chittenden has shown, however, that ptyalin acts as well, or even better, in a perfectly neutral medium. A strong alkaline reaction retards or prevents its action. The most marked influence is exerted by acids. Free hydrochloric acid to the extent of only 0.003 per cent. (Chittenden) is sufficient to practically stop the amy- lolytic action of the enzyme, and a slight further increase in acidity not only stops the action, but also destroys the enzyme. Condition of the Starch. — It is a well-known fact that the conver- sion of starch to sugar by enzymes takes place much more rapidly with cooked starch — for example, starch paste. In the latter ma- terial sugar begins to appear in a few minutes, provided a good enzyme solution is used. With starch in a raw condition, on the contrary, it may be many minutes, or even several hours, before sugar can be detected. The longer time required for raw starch is partly explained by the fact that the starch grains are surrounded by a layer of cellulose or cellulose-like material that resists the action of ptyalin. When boiled, this layer breaks and the starch in the interior becomes exposed. In addition, the starch itself is changed during the boiling ; it takes up water, and in this hydra ted condition is acted upon more rapidly by the ptyalin. The practical value of cooking vegetable foods is evident from these statements. Functions of the Saliva. — In addition to the digestive action of the saliva on starchy foods it fulfills other important functions. By moistening the food it enables us to reduce the material to a consis- tency suitable for swallowing and for manipulation by the tongue and 682 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. other muscles. Moreover, the presence of mucin serves doubtless as a kind of lubricator that insures a smooth passage along the esophageal canal. Finally by dissolving dry and solid food it pro- vides a necessary step in the process of stimulating the taste nerves, and, as is described below, the activity of the taste sensations may play an important part in the secretion of the gastric juice. CHAPTER XLIL DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE STOMACH, The muscular mechanisms by means of which the stomach is charged with food and in turn discharged, small portions at a time, into the duodenum have been described. The present chapter deals only with the chemical and mechanical changes in the food during its stay in the stomach and the extent to which the products of digestion are absorbed. The Gastric Glands. — The tubular glands that permeate the mucous membrane of the stomach throughout its entire extent differ in their histological structure, and therefore doubtless in their secre- tion, in different parts of the stomach. Two, sometimes three, kinds of glands are distinguished, — the pyloric, fundic (and cardiac). Those in the pyloric part of the stomach (antrum pylori) are char- acterized chiefly by the fact that in the secreting part of the tubule only one type of gland cell is found, the chief or peptic cell, while in the remainder of the stomach, but particularly in the middle or prepyloric region the glands (fundic glands) are distinguished by the presence of two types of cells, — the chief cells and the so-called cover or border cells (Fig. 260). The third type, the cardiac glands, is found around the cardia, but its area of distribution varies in different animals, and its histological characteristics are not very definite.* There seems to be a general agreement that the chief cells furnish the digestive enzymes of the stomach — pepsin and rennin — and the cover cells the hydrochloric acid. From a physiological standpoint it is important to remember that the cover cells are massed, as it were, in the glands of the middle or prepyloric region of the stomach, that they are scanty in the fundus, and absent in the pyloric region. This fact is indicated to the eye by the deeper red or brownish color of the mucous membrane in the prepyloric portion. Grutznerf called especial attention to this relation, and in connection with the differences in movements of these two parts of the stomach he suggests that normally the bulk of the food toward the fundus becomes impregnated first with pepsin; then, as it is slowly moved into the prepyloric region, the acid constituent is added. The pyloric glands are said (Heidenhain) to secrete an alkaline liquid containing pepsin. * See Haane, "Archiv f. Anatomie," 1905, 1. f Griitzner, " Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 106, 463, 1905. 683 684 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. -J Ifisf ological Changes in the Gastric Glands during Secretion. — TnB tells of the gastric glands, especially the so-called chief cells, show^Astinct changes as the result of prolonged activity. Upon presei'/ed specimens, taken from dogs fed at intervals of twenty-four hoiaS^ Heidenhain found that in the fasting condition the chief cells trge and clear, that during the first six hours of digestion the cells as well as the border cells increased in size, but that in a econd period, extending from the sixth to the fifteenth hour, the chief cells became gradually smaller, while the border cells remained Fig. 260.— Glands of the fundus (dog) : A and A1, during hunger, resting condition £ B, during the first stage of digestion; C and D, the second stage of digestion, showing the diminution in the size of the "chief" or central cells. — (After Heidenhain.) large or even increased in size. After the fifteenth hour the chief cells increased in size, gradually passing back to the fasting condition (see Fig. 260). Langley * has succeeded in following the changes in a more satis- factory way by observations made directly upon the living gland. He finds that the chief cells in the fasting stage are charged with granules, and that during digestion the granules are dissolved, dis- * " Journal of Physiology, " 3, 269, 1880. 685 © appearing first from the base of the cell, which then becomes filed with a non-granular material. Observations similar to those ma^^ upon other glands demonstrate that these granules represent in probability a preliminary material from which the gastric enzym are made during the act of secretion. The granules, therefore, sometimes described as zymogen granules. Means of Obtaining the Gastric Secretion and its Normal Composition. — The secretion of the gastric membrane is formed in the minute glands scattered over its surface. As there is no com- mon duct, the difficulty of obtaining the secretion for analysis or experiment is considerable. This difficulty has been overcome at different times by the invention of special methods. The older methods used for obtaining normal gastric juice were very unsatisfactory. An animal was made to swallow a clean sponge to which a string was attached so that the sponge could afterward be removed and its contents be squeezed out ; or it was made to eat some indigestible material, to start the secretion of juice by mechanical stimulation: the animal was then killed at the proper time and the contents of its stomach were collected. The experiments of the older observers on gastric digestion, especially those of the Abbe Spallanzani (1729-1799), furnish most interesting reading. Spallanzani, not content with making experiments on numerous animals (frogs, birds, mammals, etc.) had the courage to carry out a great many upon himself. He swallowed foods of various kinds and in various conditions sewed in linen bags or inclosed in perforated wooden tubes which in turn were covered with linen. The bags and tubes were subsequently passed in the stools and were examined as to the amount and nature of their contents. He seems to have experienced no injury from his experiments, although normally his powers of digestion were quite feeble. As proof that the trit- urating power of the stomach is not very great he calls attention to the fact that some of the wooden tubes were made very thin, so that the slightest Eressure would crush them, and yet they were voided uninjured. So also e found that cherries and grapes when swallowed whole, even if entirely ripe, were usually passed unbroken. A better method of obtaining normal juice was suggested by the famous observations of Beaumont* upon Alexis St. Martin. St. Martin, by the premature discharge of his gun, was wounded in the abdomen and stomach. On healing, a fistulous opening remained in the abdominal wall, leading into the stomach, so that the contents of the latter could be inspected. Beaumont made numerous inter- esting and most valuable observations upon his patient. Since that time it has become customary to make fistulous openings into the stomachs of dogs whenever it is necessary to have the normal juice for examination. A silver cannula is placed in the fistula, and at any time the plug closing the cannula may be removed and * Beaumont, "The Physiology of Digestion," 1833; second edition, 1847. For a biographical account of Beaumont, see Osier, "Journal of the American Medical Association, " November 15, 1902. 686 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. gastric juice be obtained. In some cases the esophagus has been occluded or excised so as to prevent the mixture of saliva with the gastric juice. Gastric juice may be obtained from human beings also in cases of vomiting or by means of the stomach pump, but in such cases it is necessarily more or less diluted or mixed with food and can not be used for exact analyses, although specimens of gastric juice obtained by these methods are employed in the diagnosis and treatment of gastric troubles. From the standpoint of experimental investigation a very im- portant addition to our methods was made by Heidenhain. This observer showed that a portion of the stomach — the fundic end, for instance, or the pyloric end — might be cut away from the rest of the organ and be given an artificial opening to the exterior. By this means the secretion of an isolated fundic or pyloric sac may be obtained and examined as to its quantity and prop- erties. The method was subsequently improved by Pawlow, whose important contributions are referred to below. Fig. 261 gives an idea of the operation as made by Pawlow to isolate a fundic sac with its blood and nerve supply unin- jured. The normal gastric se- cretion is a thin, colorless or nearly colorless liquid with a strong acid reaction and a characteristic odor. Its spe- cific gravity varies, but it is never great, the average being about 1.002 to 1.003. Upon analysis the gastric juice is found to contain a trace of proteid, probably a peptone, some mucin, and inorganic salts, but the essential constituents are an acid (HC1) and two enzymes, pepsin and rennin. Satisfactory complete analyses of the human juice have not been reported, most of the recent observers confining their attention mainly to the degree of acidity and digestive power. Gastric juice does not give a coagulum upon boiling, but the digestive enzymes are thereby destroyed. One of the interesting facts about this secretion is the way in which it withstands putrefac- tion. It may be kept for a long time, for months even, without becoming putrid and with very little change, if any, in its digestive Fig. 261. — To show Pawlow's operation for making an isolated fundic sac from the stomach: v, Cavity of the stomach ; s, the fundic sac, shut off from the stomach and opening at the abdominal wall, a, a; b indicates the line of sutures. — (Paw- low.) DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE STOMACH. 687 action or in its total acidity. This fact shows that the juice possesses antiseptic properties, and it is usually supposed that the presence of the free acid accounts for this quality. The Acid of Gastric Juice. — The nature of the free acid in gastric juice was formerly the subject of dispute, some claiming that the acidity is due to HC1, since this acid can be distilled off from the gas- tric juice, others contending that an organic acid, lactic acid, is present in the secretion. All recent experiments tend to prove that the acidity is due to HC1. This fact was first demonstrated satis- factorily by the analyses of Schmidt, who showed that if, in a given specimen of gastric juice, the chlorids were all precipitated by silver nitrate and the total amount of chlorin was determined, more was found than could be held in combination by the bases present in the secretion. Evidently, some of the chlorin must have been present in combination with hydrogen as hydrochloric acid. Confirmatory evidence of one kind or another has since been obtained. Thus it has been shown that a number of color tests for free mineral acids react with the gastric juice: methyl- violet solutions are turned blue, congo-red solutions and test paper are changed from red to blue, 00 tropeolin from a yellowish to a pink red, and so on. A number of additional tests of the same general character will be found described in the laboratory handbooks.* It must be added, however, that lactic acid undoubtedly occurs, or may occur, in the stomach during digestion. Its presence is usually explained as being due to the fer- mentation of the carbohydrates, and it is therefore more constantly present in the stomachs of the herbivora. The amount of free acid varies according to the duration of digestion; that is, the secretion does not possess its full acidity in the beginning, owing to the fact (Heidenhain) that in the first periods of digestion, while the secretion is still scanty in amount, a portion of its acid is neutralized by the swallowed saliva, the alkaline mucus, and the alkaline secretion of the pyloric end of the stomach; the secreted juice has, however, a constant acidity. The acidity of the human gastric juice is usually estimated at 0.3 per cent., but during digestion it may reach (Horn- borg) 0.4 to 0.5 per cent., and these figures express probably its strength as secreted. The acidity of the dog's gastric juice, according to Pawlow, lies between 0.46 and 0.56 per cent. The Origin of the HC1. — The gastric juice is the only secretion of the body that contains a free acid. The fact that the acid is a mineral acid and is present in considerable strength makes the cir- cumstance more remarkable. Attempts have been made to ascer- tain the histological elements concerned in its secretion and the nature of the chemical reaction or reactions by which it is produced. With regard to the first point it is generally believed that the border * Simon, " A Manual of Clinical Diagnosis, " 1904. 688 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. cells of the gastric tubules constitute the acid-secreting cells. This belief is founded upon the general fact that in the regions in which these cells are chiefly present — that is, the middle region of the stomach — the secretion is distinctly acid, and where they are absent or scanty in number the secretion is alkaline or less acid. In the pyloric region, for instance, these cells are lacking entirely and the secretion is alkaline. So also in the fundus the secretion does not seem to be acid, and this fact corresponds with a marked diminution or absence of the border cells. With regard to the origin of the acid it is evident that it is formed in the secreting cells, since none exists in the blood or lymph. It seems also perfectly evident that the HC1 must be formed from the chlorids of the blood. The chief chlorid is NaCl and by some means this compound is broken up; the chlorin is combined with hydrogen, and is then secreted upon the free surface of the stomach as HC1. In support of this general statement it has been shown that if the chlorids in the blood are reduced by removing them from the food for a sufficient time the secretion of gastric juice no longer contains acid. On the other hand, addition of NaBr or KI to the food may cause the formation of some HBr and HI, toge her with HC1 in the gastric juice. Maly has suggested that acid phosphates may be produced in the first instance, and then by reacting with the sodium chlorid may give hydrochloric acid, accord- ing to the formula NaH2P04 + NaCl = Na2HP04 + HC1. Other theories have been proposed, but, as a matter of fact, no explanation of the details of this reaction is satisfactory. We must be content to say that in the acid-forming cells the neutral chlorids (NaCl) are broken up with the formation of free HC1. The Secretory Nerves of the Gastric Glands. — Although several facts indicated to the older observers that the secretion of gastric juice is under the control of nerve fibers, we owe the actual experi- mental demonstration of this fact to Pawlow.* He demonstrated that the secretion is under the control of the nervous system and that the secretory fibers are contained in the vagus. Direct stimulation of the peripheral end of the cut vagus causes a secretion of gastric juice after a long latent period of several minutes. This long latency may be due possibly to the presence in the vagus of inhibitory fibers to the gland, which, being stimulated simultaneously with the secretory fibers, delay the action of the latter. Very striking proof of the general fact that the secretion is due to the action of vagus fibers is furnished by such experiments as these : Pawlow divided the esophagus in the neck and brought the two ends to the skin so as to make separate fistulous openings to the exterior. Under these con- ditions, when the animal ate and swallowed food it was discharged * See Pawlow, " The Work of the Digestive Glands, " translated by Thompson, 1902. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE STOMACH. 689 to the exterior instead of entering the stomach. The animal thus had the enjoyment of eating without actually filling the stomach. Eating in this style forms what the author called a fictitious meal (Schein- filtterung). It was found that it causes an abundant flow of gastric juice as long as the vagi are intact, but has no effect on the secretion when these nerves are cut. Evidently, therefore, the sensations of taste, odor, etc., developed during the mastication and swallowing of food set up reflexly a stimulation of secretory fibers in the vagus. Pawlow designates a secretion produced in this way as a psychical secretion, — a term which implies that the reflex must be attended by conscious sensations. In favorable cases the fictitious feeding has been continued for five or six hours and a large amount of gastric juice (700 c.c.) has been collected from a fistula, although no food actually entered the stomach. It is important to note, also, that a psychical secretion, once started, may continue for a long time after the stimulus (the eating) has ceased. Experiments have been made upon human beings under similar conditions. Thus, Hornborg* reports the case of a boy with a stricture of the esophagus and a fistula in the stomach. Food when chewed and swallowed did not reach the stomach, but was regurgitated; it caused, nevertheless, an active psychical secretion in the empty stomach. Normal Mechanism of the Secretion of the Gastric Juice. — During a meal the gastric juice is secreted, under normal conditions, as long as the food remains in the stomach. The modern explana- tion of the origin, maintenance, and regulation of this flow of secre- tion is that given by Pawlow. Contrary to a former general belief, he showed that mechanical stimulation of the gastric mucous mem- brane has no effect on the secretion of the tubules. This factor may therefore be eliminated. In an ordinary meal the secretion first started is due to the sensations of eating — that is, it is a psychical secretion. The afferent stimuli originate in the mouth and nostrils; the efferent path, the secretory fibers, is through the vagus nerve. This reflex insures the beginning at least of gastric digestion, but its effect is supplemented by reflex stimulations arising in the stomach itself. The mucous membrane of the stomach contains sensory fibers which, when stimulated, act reflexly upon the secretory fibers and set up a secretion. It seems that some foods contain substances capable of giving this effect, while others do not. Thus, meat extracts, meat juices, soups, etc., are particularly effective in this respect; milk and water cause less secretion. Certain common articles of food, such as bread and white of eggs, have no effect of this kind at all. If introduced into the stomach of a dog through a fistula so as not to arouse a psychical secretion, — for instance, while the dog's attention is diverted or while he is sleeping, — they cause no flow of * Hornborg, " Skandinavisches Archiv f. Physiologic," 15, 209, 1904! 44 690 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. gastric juice and are not digested. If such articles of food are eaten, however, they cause a psychical secretion, and when this has acted upon the foods some product of their digestion in turn becomes ca- pable of stimulating the secretory paths by a reflex from the stomach. The steps in the mechanism of secretion are, therefore, three: (1) the psychical secretion; (2) the secretion from secretogogues contained in the food; (3) the secretion from secretogogues contained in the products of digestion. The chemical nature of these last-named stimuli is undeter- mined. The researches of Pawlow and his co- workers seem also to indicate that the quantity and prop- erties of the secre- tion vary with the character of the food. The quantity of the secretion varies, also, other conditions be- ing the same, with the amount of food to be digested. The apparatus is adjusted in this respect to work economically. Different kinds of food produce secre- tions varying not only as regards quan- tity, but also in their acidity and digestive action. The secre- tion produced by bread, though less in quantity than that caused by meat, possesses a greater digestive action. On a given diet the secretion assumes certain characteris- tics, and Pawlow is convinced that further work will disclose the fact that the secretion of the stomach is not caused normally by general stimuli all affecting it alike, but by specific stimuli contained in the food or produced during digestion, whose action is of such a kind as to arouse reflexly the secretion best adapted to the food ingested. id is o "8 t-i a » s \ s i \ i \ i \ ^ / 10 V 8 \ 6 \ 4 V 2 k i . i f \ 0 Hours / ZS4£6Y8? W1T1I Quantity of secretion. Acidity. Digestive power. Fig. 262. — Diagram showing the variation in quantity of gastric secretion in the dog after a mixed meal; also the variations in acidity and in digestive power. — (After Khigine.) DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE STOMACH. 691 One of the curves, showing the effect of a mixed diet (milk, 600 c.c; meat, 100 gms.; bread, 100 gms.) upon the gastric secretion, as determined by Pawlow's method, is reproduced in Fig. 262. It will be noticed that the secretion began shortly after the ingestion of the food (seven minutes), and increased rapidly to a maximum that was reached in two hours. After the second hour the flow decreased rapidly and nearly uniformly to about the tenth hour. The acidity rose slightly between the first and second hours, and then fell gradu- ally. The digestive power showed an increase between the second and third hours. Nature and Properties of Pepsin. — Pepsin is a typical proteo- lytic enzyme that exhibits the striking peculiarity of acting only in acid media; hence peptic digestion in the stomach is the result of the combined action of pepsin and hydrochloric acid. Pepsin is influenced in its action by temperature, as is the case with the other enzymes; low temperatures retard, and may even suspend its activity, while high temperatures increase it. The optimum tem- perature is stated to be from 37° to 40° C, while exposure for some time to 80° C. results, when the pepsin is in a moist condition, in the total destruction of the enzyme. Pepsin may be extracted from the gastric mucous membrane by a variety of methods and in different degrees of purity and strength. The commercial preparations of pepsin consist usually of some form of extract of the gastric mucous membrane to which starch or sugar of milk has been added. Labora- tory preparations are made conveniently by mincing thoroughly the mucous membrane and then extracting for a long time with glycerin. Glycerin extracts, if not too much diluted with water or blood, keep for an indefinite time. Purer preparations of pepsin have been made by what is known as "Brucke's method/' in which the mucous membrane is minced and is then self-digested with a 5 per cent, solution of phosphoric acid. The phosphoric acid is pre- cipitated by the addition of lime-water, and the pepsin is carried down in the flocculent precipitate. This precipitate, after being washed, is carried into solution by dilute hydrochloric acid, and a solution of cholesterin in alcohol and ether is added. The cholesterin is precipitated, and, as before, carries down with it the pepsin. This precipitate is collected, carefully washed, and then treated repeatedly with ether, which dissolves and removes the cholesterin, leaving the pepsin in aqueous solution. This method is interesting not only because it gives a pure form of pepsin, but also in that it illustrates one of the properties of enzymes — namely, the readiness with which they adhere to precipitates occurring in their solutions. In spite of much work the chemical nature of pepsin is undeter- mined. Most authors agree that it is a proteid or proteid-containing 692 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. body. Pekelharing* has prepared pepsin from gastric juice by dialysis, the substance precipitating as the acid is dialyzed off. The precipitate may be purified by repeated resolutions in acid followed by dialysis. As prepared by this method pepsin is a substance of a proteid nature which contains sulphur and also some chlorin, but no phosphorus. It does not belong, therefore, to the group of nucleo- proteids. Pepsin is supposed to be formed in the chief cells of the gastric tubules, but as in other cases it is present in the cells as a zymogen or propepsin which is not changed to the active pepsin until after secretion. The propepsin may be extracted readily from the mucous membrane, and, since it is known that the zymogen is converted quickly to active pepsin by the action of acids, it is evident that in the normal gastric juice the existence of the hydrochloric acid insures that all of- the pepsin shall be present in active form. There has been much discussion as to the nature of the secretion of the pyloric glands. Heidenhain isolated this portion of the stomach and collected its secretion. He found that it was alkaline and contained pepsin. Later observers, however, still continue to doubt the secre- tion of a true pepsin in this portion of the stomach. Glaessnerf states that propepsin can not be obtained from extracts of the pyloric glands, and that the proteolytic enzyme that can be shown in this portion of the stomach by self-digestion in acid or alkaline media is not a true gastric pepsin. Whether the pyloric glands take any chemical part in gastric digestion must remain undecided. From the description of the events in the stomach (p. 641) it would seem that the food material which is churned and stirred by the contrac- tions of the pyloric musculature has already been charged with pepsin and hydrochloric acid by the glands of the middle and fundic regions. Artificial Gastric Juice. — In studying peptic digestion it is not necessary for all purposes to establish a gastric fistula. The active agents of the normal juice are pepsin and acid of a proper strength; and, as the pepsin can be extracted and preserved in various ways, and the hydrochloric acid can easily be made of the proper strength, an artificial juice can be obtained at any time which may be used in place of the normal secretion for many purposes. In laboratory experiments it is customary to employ a glycerin extract of the gastric mucous membrane, and to add a small portion of this extract to a large bulk of 0.2 per cent, hydrochloric acid. The artificial juice thus made, when kept at a temperature of from 37° to 40° C, will digest proteids rapidly if the preparation of pepsin is a good one. While the strength of the acid employed is generally from 0.2 to 0.3 per cent., digestion will take place in solutions of greater * Pekelharing, "Zeitschrift f. physiol. Chemie," 35, 8, 1902. t Glaessner, " Beitrage zur chem. Physiol, u. Pathol.," 1, 24, 1901. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE STOMACH. 693 or less acidity. Too great or too small an acidity, however, will retard the process; that is, there is for the action of the pepsin an optimum acidity which lies somewhere between 0.2 and 0.5 per cent. Other acids may be used in place of the hydrochloric acid — for example, nitric, phosphoric, or lactic — but they are not so effective, and the optimum acidity is different for each; for phos- phoric acid it is given as 2 per cent. The Pepsin-Hydrochloric Acid Digestion of Proteids. — It has long been known that solid proteids, when exposed to the action of a normal or an artificial gastric juice, swell up and eventually pass into solution. The soluble proteid thus formed was known not to be coagulated by heat and was remarkable also for being more diffusible than other forms of soluble proteids. This end-product of digestion was formerly conceived as a soluble proteid with properties fitting it for rapid absorption, and the name of peptone was given to it. It was quickly found, however, that the process is complicated — that in the conversion to so-called "peptone" the proteid under digestion passes through a number of intermediate stages. The intermediate products were partially isolated and were given specific names, such as acid-albumin, parapeptone, and propeptone. The present concep- tion of the process we owe chiefly to Kiihne. This author believed that the proteid passes through three general stages before reaching the final condition of peptone. This view is indicated briefly by the following schema : Native proteid. Acid albumin (syntonin) . Primary proteoses (protalbumoses). Secondary proteoses (deutero-albumoses). Peptone. The first step is the conversion of the proteid to an acid albumin. This change may be considered as being chiefly an effect of the hy- drochloric acid, although in some way the combined action of the pepsin-hydrochloric acid compound is more effective than a solution of the acid alone of the same strength. Like the acid albumins in general (see appendix), the syntonin is readily precipitated on neutralization. In the beginning of peptic digestion, therefore, if the solution is neutralized with dilute alkali, an abundant precipi- tate of syntonin occurs. Later on in the digestion neutralization gives no such effect — the syntonin has all passed into a further stage of digestion. Under the influence of the pepsin the syntonin under- goes hydrolysis, with the production of a number of bodies which as a group are designated as primary proteoses or protalbumoses.* * The products intermediate between the original proteid and the pep- tone are described in general as albumoses or as proteoses, according as one takes the term proteid or albumin as the generic name for the original sub- stance. The term proteid is generally used in English ; hence, the intermedi- ate products are more appropriately designated as proteoses. 694 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. Although several members of this group have been isolated and given separate names, so much doubt prevails as to the chemical individ- uality of these substances that it is best perhaps to regard them as a group of compounds which under the continued influence of the pepsin undergo still further hydrolysis with the formation of secon- dary proteoses or deutero-albumoses. As compared with the primary proteoses, the secondary ones are distinguished by a greater solu- bility ; they require a stronger saturation with neutral salts to precipi- tate them. (See appendix.) The secondary proteids undergo still further hydrolysis, with the production of peptone, or perhaps it would be better to say peptones. The peptones show still greater solubility, and, in fact, peptone, in Kuhne's sense, is that compound or group of compounds formed in peptic digestion which, while still showing proteid reactions (biuret reaction), is not coagulated by heat nor precipitated when its solutions are completely saturated with ammonium sulphate. According to the schema and descrip- tion given above, the several stages in peptic digestion are repre- sented as following in sequence. It should be stated, however, that many authors consider that even in the beginning of the digestion the proteid molecule may be split into several complexes, and that some of the end-products may be formed in the very beginning of the action. All that we can state very positively is that the proteid molecules undergo a series of hydrolytic cleav- ages, the end-result of which is that in place of the originally very large molecule with a weight of 5000 to 7000 there is obtained a number of much smaller and much more soluble molecules whose molecular weights are perhaps only 250 to 400 or less. It was formerly believed that pepsin was not able to split the complex proteid molecule into compounds of a simpler structure than the peptone. But a number of recent authors have stated that if time enough is given the breaking up of the proteid molecule may be as complete as after the action of trypsin, or after hydrolysis by acids (see Proteids in appendix). That is, along with the peptone or in place of it are found certain simpler bodies which no longer give the biuret reaction, but are precipitable by phospho- tungstic acid and for which Hofmeister proposes the general name of pep- toids. They would correspond, also, apparently, to the group of compounds designated by Fischer as peptids or polypeptids. In addition, many of the amido-acids and nitrogenous bases which constitute the final end-products of the breaking up of the proteid molecule may be found.* In judging the digestive action of any given specimen of natural or artificial gastric juice it is customary to measure the rapidity with which an insoluble proteid is converted into a soluble form. The method most commonly employed is that devised in Pawlow's laboratory by Mett. The Mett test is made by sucking white of egg into a thin-walled glass tube having an internal diameter of 1 to 2 mms. The egg-albumin is coagulated in the tube by immersing it for * See Hofmeister, " Ergebnisse der Physiologic," vol. i, part i, 796, 1902. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE STOMACH. 695 five minutes in water at 95° C. After some time the tube is cut into lengths of 10 to 15 rams, and these are used to test the digestive action or amount of pepsin. One or more of the tubes are placed in the solution to be measured and kept for ten hours at body temperature. The digestive power is measured in terms of the length in millimeters of the column of egg-albumin that is dissolved. The relative amounts of pepsin in solutions compared in this way are determined by the law of Schiitz, according to which the digestive power is proportional to the square root of the amount of pepsin. If in two specimens of gastric juice the number of millimeters of egg albumin digested was in one case two and in the other three, the digestive powers of the two solutions would be as the squares of the numbers, as 4 to 9. The Rennin Enzyme (Rennet, Chymosin). — The property possessed by the mucous membrane of the calf's stomach of curdling milk has been known from remote times, and has been utilized in the manufacture of cheeze and curds. This action takes place with remarkable rapidity under favorable conditions, a large mass of milk setting to a firm coagulum within a very brief time. It has been shown that this action is due to an enzyme,— rennin or rennet. The rennin, like the pepsin, is supposed to be formed in the chief cells of the gastric tubules and to be present in the glands in a zymogen form, the prorennin or prochymosin, which after secretion is con- verted to the active enzyme. This conversion takes place very readily under the influence of acid. Rennin (or its zymogen) may be obtained easily from the mucous membrane of the stomach (with the exception of the pyloric end) by extracting with glycerin or water or by digesting it with dilute acid. Good extracts of rennin cause the milk to clot with great rapidity at a temperature of 40° C, the milk (cows' milk), if undisturbed setting first into a solid clot, which afterward shrinks and presses out a clear, yellowish liquid — the whey. With human milk the curd is much less firm, and takes the form of loose flocculi. The whole process resembles much the clotting of blood. The rapidity of clotting is said to vary inversely as the amount of rennin, or, in other words, the product of the amount of rennin and the time necessary for clotting is a constant. The curdling of the milk involves two apparently independent proc- esses : First, the rennin acts upon the casein of the milk and converts it into a substance known as paracasein. The paracasein then reacts with the lime salts of the milk, forming an insoluble calcium salt, which constitutes the curd or coagulum. According to this view, the enzyme does not cause clotting directly. What takes place when the casein is changed to paracasein is not understood. Ham- marsten originally regarded the change as a cleavage process, but this view has not been supported. Others have supposed that a transformation or rearrangement of molecular structure occurs. 696 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. Indeed, the differences in properties between casein and paracasein are not great, the most marked difference being that the calcium salts of the latter are insoluble. If soluble calcium salts are removed from milk by the addition of oxalate solutions it does not curdle upon the addition of rennin. Addition of lime salts restores this property. It should be added that casein is also precipitated from milk by the addition of an excess of acid. The curdling of sour milk in the formation of bonnyclabber is a well-known illustration of this fact. When milk stands for some time the action of bacteria upon the milk- sugar leads to the formation of lactic acid, and when this acid reaches a certain concentration it causes the precipitation of the casein. So far as our positive knowledge goes, the action of rennin is confined to milk. Casein is the chief proteid constituent of milk, and has therefore an important nutritive value. It is interesting to find that before its peptic digestion begins the casein is acted upon by an altogether different enzyme. The value of the curdling action is not at once apparent, but we may suppose that casein is more easily digested under the conditions that exist in the body after it has been brought into a solid form. This has, however, been doubted, and it has even been suggested that the process is a hin- drance rather than an aid to the digestion of the casein. Until the contrary is definitely demonstrated it is preferable to assume that the process is of importance in the digestion of milk. The action of rennin goes no further than the curdling; the digestion of the curd is carried on by the pepsin, and later, in the intestines, by the trypsin, with the formation of proteoses and peptones as in the case of other proteids.* Rennin is found elsewhere than in the gastric mucosa. It has been described in the pancreatic juice, in the testis, and in many other organs as well as in the tissues of many plants. In fact, wherever proteolytic enzymes are found there also some evidence of a curdling action on milk may be ob- tained. For this reason some observers f have taken the view that the milk coag- ulation is not due to a specific ferment, but is an action of the pepsin itself. That is, the proteolytic enzyme is capable of causing the change from casein to paracasein as well as the hydrolysis of the proteid. This view is opposed to the prevalent opinion regarding the specificity of enzyme actions. Another interesting fact concerning rennin is that an animal may be im- munized against it (see p. 387). If rennin be injected subcutaneously in an animal an antirennin will be formed in its blood. This antirennin added to milk prevents its curdling by rennin, giving a result, therefore, similar to the reaction between toxins and antitoxins. The Digestive Changes Undergone by the Food in the Stomach. — In addition to the pepsin and rennin various observers * For references to the very abundant literature consult Oppenheimer, loc. cit. f See Pawlow and Parastschuk, " Zeitschrift f . physiol. Chemie, " 42, 415. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE STOMACH. 697 have described other enzymes in the gastric juice or gastric mem- brane,— a lipase or fat-splitting enzyme (Volhard), an amylolytic or starch-splitting enzyme (Friedenthal), and an inverting enzyme (Widdicombe) , but the normal existence or at least the normal action of these latter enzymes in digestion is a matter about which little is known. As was said above, it is probable that the ptyalin swallowed with the food continues to exert its action upon the starchy materials in the fundus for a long time, so that in this way the starch digestion in the stomach may be important. Regarding the fats, it is usually believed that they undergo no truly digestive change in the stomach. They are set free from their intimate mixture with other food- stuffs by the dissolving action of the gastric juice upon proteids, they are liquefied by the heat of the body, and they are disseminated through the chyme in a coarse emulsion by the movements of the stomach. In this way they are mechanically prepared so that the subsequent action of the pancreatic juice is much favored. When, however, fats are ingested in emulsified form, as in milk, for instance, the lipase of the stomach, according to Volhard, may cause a marked hydrolysis. It is supposed that this action may be impor- tant in the digestion of the milk-fat by infants. Regarding the proteids, the practical point of interest is how far they are digested during their stay in the stomach. It seems probable that this question does not admit of a categorical answer, — that is, the extent of the digestion varies under different circumstances, with the consistency of the food, the duration of its stay in the stomach, etc. In the liquid material (chyme) forced through the pylorus into the duodenum one may find unchanged proteids, primary or secondary proteoses, peptones, or even the final split products of proteolytic action. The true value of peptic digestion is not so much in its own action as in its combined action with the trypsin of the pancreatic juice. The digestion of the proteids of the food is effected by both enzymes, and normally we are justified in considering them together as forming a peptic-try ptic digestion. The preliminary digestion in the stomach is important as regards the proteid foods from several standpoints: First, in the matter of mechanical preparation of the food and its discharge in convenient quantities easily handled by the duodenum. Second, in the more or less complete hydrolysis to peptones and proteoses whereby the action of the pancreatic juice must be greatly accelerated. Indeed, in some cases this preliminary action of the pepsin-hydrochloric acid may be absolutely necessary. Native proteids, such as serum- albumin, are not acted upon by trypsin, but if submitted first to pepsin-hydrochloric acid they are quickly digested by this enzyme. Third, for some as yet unknown reason proteids submitted to peptic digestion are split by the trypsin in a way different from its action 698 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. on proteids without this preliminary treatment. These and other facts seem to indicate that the peptic digestion is not so much an end in itself as a preparation for subsequent intestinal digestion. The stomach, therefore, may be removed without a fatal result. Several cases are on record in which the stomach was practically removed by surgical operation, the esophagus being stitched to the duodenum.* The animals did well and seemed perfectly normal, although special precautions were necessary in the matter of feeding. Absorption in the Stomach. — In the stomach it is possible that there may be absorption of the following substances : Water; salts; sugars and dextrins that may have been formed in salivary digestion from starch, or that may have been eaten as such; the proteoses and peptones formed in the peptic digestion of proteids or albuminoids. In addition, absorption of soluble or liquid substances — drugs, alcohol, etc., that have been swallowed — may occur. It was formerly assumed, without definite proof, that the stomach absorbs easily such things as water, salts, sugars, and peptones. Actual experi- ments, however, made, under conditions as nearly normal as possible, show, upon the whole, that absorption does not take place readily in the stomach — certainly nothing like so easily as in the intestine. The methods made use of in these experiments have varied, but the most interesting results have been obtained by establishing a fistula of the duodenum just beyond the pylorus.f After establishing this fistula food may be given to the animal and the contents of the stomach as they pass out. through the pyloric opening may be caught and examined. Water. — Experiments of the character just described show that water when taken alone is practically jiot-absorbed at all in the stomach. Von Mering's experiments especially show that as soon as water is introduced into the stomach it begins to pass into the intestine, being forced out in a series bf spurts by the contractions of the stomach. Within a comparatively short time practically all the water can be recovered in this way, none or very little having been absorbed in the stomach. For example, in a large dog with a fistula in the duodenum, 500 c.c. of water were given through the mouth. Within twenty-five minutes 495 c.c. had been forced out of the stomach through the duodenal fistula. This result is not true for all liquids; alcohol, for example, is absorbed readily. Salts. — The absorption of salts from the stomach has not been investigated thoroughly. According to Brandl, sodium iodid is absorbed very slowly or not at all in dilute solutions. Not until its *Ludwig and Ogata, "Archiv f. Physiologie, " 1883, p. 89; Carvallo and Pachon, "Archives de physiologie norm, et path.," 1894, p. 106. t Compare von Mering, " Verhandl. des Congresses f. innere Med.," 12, 471, 1893; Edkins, " Journal of Physiology," 13, 445, 1892; Brandl, " Zeitschrift f. Biologie," 29, 277, 1892. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE STOMACH. 699 solutions reach a concentration of 3 per cent, or more does its absorp- tion become important. This result, if applicable to all the soluble inorganic salts, would indicate that under ordinary conditions they are prfl.nt.infl.lly not absorbed ^ t^ st.nmfl.nh, since it can not be sup- posed that they are normally swallowed in solutions so concentrated as 3 per cent. In the same direction Meltzer reports that solutions of strychnin are absorbed with difficulty from the stomach as com- pared with the intestines, rectum, or even the pharynx. It is said that the absorption of sodium iodid is very much facilitated by the use of condiments, such as mustard and pepper, or alcohol, which act either by causing a greater congestion of the mucous membrane or perhaps by directly stimulating the epithelial cells. Sugars and Peptones. — Experiments by the newer methods leave no doubt that sugars and peptones can be absorbed from the stomach. In von Mering's work different forms of sugar — dextrose, laclpse, saccharose (cane-sugar), maltose, and also dextrin — were tested. They were all absorbed, but it was found that absorption was more marked the more concentrated were the solutions. Brandl reports that sugar (dextrose) and peptone are not sensibly absorbed until the concentration has reached 5 per cent. With these substances also the ingestion of condiments or of alcohol increases distinctly the absorptive processes in the stomach. Examination of the mucous membrane of a stomach in full digestion shows that it contains albumoses (Glaessner), — a fact that indicates some absorption. Direct examination of the stomach contents* indicates that the products of peptic action beyond the albumose stage — namely, the peptones, peptids, and amido-bodies — are absorbed. On the whole, however, it would seem that sugars and peptones are ab- sorbed with some difficulty from the stomach. Fats. — As we have seen, fats probably undergo no digestive changes in the stomach. The processes of saponification and emulsification are supposed to be preliminary steps to absorption, and, as these processes take place after the fats have reached the small intestine, there seems to be no doubt that in the stomach fats as usually ingested escape absorption. * Zimz, "Beitrage zur chem. Physiol, u. Pathol.," 3. 339, 1903. CHAPTER XLIII. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE INTESTINES. The food undergoes its most profound digestive changes in the intestines, and here also the products of digestion are mainly ab- sorbed. The intestinal digestion begins in the duodenum, and is largely completed by the time that the food arrives at the ileocecal valve. It is effected through the combined action of three secretions, — the pancreatic juice, the secretion from the intestinal glands (succus entericus), and the bile. These secretions are mixed with the food from the duodenum on, so that their action proceeds simultaneously. For purposes of description it is necessary to speak of each more or less separately. The Pancreas. — The pancreas forms a long, narrow gland reach- ing from the spleen to the curvature of the duodenum. Its main duct in man (duct of Wirsung) opens into the duodenum, together with the common bile-duct, about 8 to 10 cms. beyond the pylorus. The points at which the duct or ducts of the pancreas enter the intestine vary somewhat in different mammals. In the dog there are two ducts, one opening, together with the bile-duct, about 3 to 5 cms. below the pylorus, while a second enters the duodenum some 3 to 5 cms. farther down. In rabbits the principal pancreatic duct opens separately into the duodenum about 35 cms. below the opening of the bile-duct. The pancreas is a compound tubular gland like the salivary glands. The cells lining the secreting portion of the tubules, the alveoli, belong to the serous or albuminous type. They are characterized by the fact that the outer portion of each cell is composed of a clear, non-granular material which stains readily, while the inner portion, the portion facing the lumen, contains numerous granules. Histological study of the gland after active secretion, as compared with the resting state, has shown very con- clusively that these granules represent a preparatory material for secretion. As the secretion proceeds the granules are dissolved and discharged into the lumen, while during the periods of rest new granules are formed by metabolic processes at the expense, appar- ently, of the non-granular material in the basal portion of the cell. (Heidenhain, Kuhne, Lea). The histological picture of secretion is in general the same in this as in the salivary and gastric glands, only somewhat more distinctly shown. On the supposition that the granules constitute an antecedent material from which the enzymes 700 DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE INTESTINES. 701 of the secretion are formed they are frequently designated as zymogen granules. The pancreas contains also certain peculiar groups of cells, the islands (or bodies) of Langerhans. These cells have noth- ing to do with the digestive activity of the pancreas. Their function is referred to in the sections on internal secretions and nutrition. Composition of the Secretion. — The pancreatic secretion is an alkaline liquid which in some animals is thin and limpid, in others thick and glairy. The secretion in man belongs to the former type; it is described as water-clear and as having a specific gravity of 1.0075. The secretion may be collected by opening the abdomen and inserting a cannula directly into the duct, or a permanent fistula may be made by the method of Pawlow. This method, applicable to the dog, consists in cutting out a small portion of the duodenum where the pancreatic duct opens and then suturing this piece, with mucous membrane outward, into the abdominal wall. The secretion in this case pours out upon the exterior and may be collected. The animal, however, suffers nutritive disturbances from the loss of the secretion, and requires careful dieting and atten- tion. The secretion of the human pancreas has been collected in a single case* in which for a few days it was necessary to drain off the pancreatic juice to the exterior. From the observations made in this case it appears that the secretion in man is quite abundant, amount- ing to 500 to 800 c.c. per day. In the cow (Delezenne) from 1J to 2 liters may be collected in the course of a day. The secretion possesses a strong alkaline reaction, due to the presence of sodium carbonate; it contains also a small amount of coagulable proteid and a number of organic substances in traces. The important constituents, how- ever, are three enzymes or their zymogens, — namely, trypsin, a proteolytic enzyme; pancreatic diastase (amylopsin), an amylolytic enzyme; and lipase (steapsin), a lipolytic enzyme. Some authors state, also, that the secretion contains a rennin enzyme. Glaessner reports that he got no evidence of this last enzyme in human pan- creatic juice. Secretory Nerve Fibers to the Pancreas. — The pancreas receives its nerve supply immediately from the celiac plexus, but stimulation of the nerves going to this plexus — namely, the splanch- nics and the vagi — gave negative results in the hands of most observers so far as the pancreatic secretion was concerned. Pawlowf and his coworkers, however, have been more successful. Mechanical stimulation or electrical stimulation of the vagus or splanchnic gave them a marked flow of pancreatic juice, but when the latter form of * See Glaessner, " Zeitschrift f. physiol. Chemie, " 40, 465, 1903. t For recent work upon the pancreas and the literature see Pawlow, "The Work of the Digestive Glands," translation by Thompson, 1902; Bay- liss and Starling, "Journal of Physiology," 30, 61, 19^4^\^SaJ^t*r^-Archives des sciences biologiques, " 7, 1, 1899. LIERaRY 702 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. stimulus was used it was necessary to cut the splanchnic some days previously in order that the vasoconstrictor fibers might degenerate. It seems that the secretory activity of the gland is prevented when there is an interference with its blood supply. In this respect the pancreas differs from the salivary glands. The secretion obtained upon stimulation of the nerves is characterized, as in the case of the gastric glands, by a long latent period of some minutes, — a fact that is explained, although not satisfactorily, on the assumption that the nerve trunks stimulated contain both secretory and inhibitory fibers and that the antagonistic action of the latter delays the appearance of the secretion. These observations are usually taken as proof of the 1. %. 3. u. s. 6. 7. 1. At / \ ho / / I \ SZ A s. -V \ /;/ V. •n U ■ / \ ■; Iff \ \ \ /6 m it X \' \ ii / \ \^ | / / \ \ \ kv I! / — " "■ s 0 1 / *— — . / N s ^^^ * / y .Xiltc. .(Wd. .Meat. Fig. 263. — Four curves of the secretion of the pancreatic juice, the three in black, from Walter, showing the secretion in dogs on different diets: (1) on 600 c.c. of milk; (2) on 250 gms. bread; (3) on 100 gms. of meat. The curve in red, from Glaessner, shows the secretion in man on a mixed diet, soup, meat, and bread. The figures, 1, 2, 3 etc., along the abscissa indicate hours after the beginning of the meal. The figures along the ordinates indicate the quantity of the secretion in cubic centimeters. existence of secretory nerve fibers to the pancreas; but, as will be explained below, it is not yet clear how far these fibers are con- cerned in arousing the flow of secretion during digestion. The Curve of Secretion. — The rate of flow of the pancreatic juice with reference to the period of digestion has been determined by a number of observers. In the careful experiments reported by Walter it is shown that the quantity of secretion is dependent to a considerable extent upon the character of the food. Thus, the flow is more abundant and reaches its maximum sooner after a meal of bread alone than after a meal of meat alone. It seems possible that the latter point, the time at which the maximum flow DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE INTESTINES. 703 is reached, may depend upon the difference in rate at which these foods are ejected from the stomach. Cannon (p. 642) has shown that the carbohydrate foods leave the stomach sooner than the proteids or fats. It is stated, however, that the composition of the secretion varies also with the character of the food, and indeed shows an adaptation to the character of the food. The secretion caused by proteid food is especially rich in trypsin, that caused by fatty food in lipase, etc. The mechanism by which this adaptation is secured is not understood. Glaessner* has measured the rate of flow in man, and his curve for a mixed diet is represented also (in red) in Fig. 263. These curves indicate in general that the secretion of pancreatic juice begins very soon after food enters the stomach, and increases rapidly to a maximum, which is reached somewhere between the second and fourth hour. According to Glaessner's case, there is a continuous small secretion of the juice during fast- ing. The observations on dogs, on the contrary, indicate an entire cessation of the flow when the stomach is empty. Boldirefff has reported a very curious activity of the digestive organs during fasting. It seems that (in dogs) when the stomach or even the small intestine is empty the entire gastro-intestinal canal exhibits periodical out- breaks of activity, which occur at intervals of two hours and last for twenty to thirty minutes. During this stage the stomach and intestines exhibit movements, and there is an abundant secretion of pancreatic juice, bile, and intestinal juice, which is subsequently absorbed. Acids introduced into the stomach or intestines prevent the occurrence of these periods, and they are absent, therefore, as long as the stomach contains gastric juice. The author's suggestion that the secretions thus formed furnish active enzymes which are absorbed into the blood and utilized by the tissues in destroying the newly absorbed food does not commend itself as probable. Normal Mechanism of the Pancreatic Secretion — Secretin. — Much light was thrown upon the mechanism of pancreatic secretion by the discovery (Dolinsky, 1895) that acids brought into contact with the mucous membrane of the duodenum set up promptly a secretion of pancreatic juice. Since this discovery it has been be- lieved that the acid gastric juice is the means that serves to inaugurate the flow from the pancreas. As soon as any of the acid contents of the stomach pass through the pylorus this action begins. Just as the chewing and swallowing of the food initiate the gastric secretion, so the acid of the latter starts the pancreatic secretion. Inasmuch as the pancreatic gland possesses secretory fibers it was assumed at first that the acid acts reflexly through these fibers, — that is, the acid in the duodenum acting upon sensory endings causes a reflex stimu- lation of the efferent secretory fibers. It has been stated, however, that the same effect takes place after section of the vagus and splanchnic nerves (Popielski),and Bayliss and StarlingJ have called * Glaessner, loc.cit. t Boldireff, "Archives des sciences biologiques, " 11, 1, 1905. j Bayliss and Starling, " Journal of Physiology, " 28, 325, 1902. 704 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. attention to another more probable explanation. These authors find that if the mucous membrane of the duodenum (or jejunum) is scraped off and treated with acid (0.4 per cent. HC1) the extract thus made when injected into the blood sets up an active secretion of pancreatic juice. They have shown that this effect is due to a special substance, secretin, which is formed by the action of the acid upon some substance (prosecretin) present in the mucous membrane. Secretin is not an enzyme, since its activity is not destroyed by boil- ing or by the action of alcohol. The experimental evidence at present favors the view that the normal sequence of events is as follows : The acid of the gastric juice upon reaching the duodenum produces secretin; this in turn is absorbed by the blood, carried to the pancreas, and stimulates this organ to activity. Whether the acid in addition causes reflex stimulation of the secretory nerves is not certain, although this statement is made. It is not clear, also, by what means the secretion of pancreatic juice is maintained through the six or seven hours or more of gastro-intestinal digestion. The increased flow during the first hours is connected with the increas- ing discharge of food from stomach to intestine, but whether the effect upon the pancreas is traceable entirely to a continual forma- tion of secretin, or partly to secretin and partly to nerve stimulation, are matters not yet settled. Enterokinase. — It was discovered in Pawlow's laboratory (Chepowalnikow) that the pancreatic juice obtained from a fistula may have little or no digestive action on proteids, but if brought into contact with the duodenal membrane or an extract of this membrane it shows at once powerful proteolytic properties. This discovery has been confirmed repeatedly. Evidently the proteolytic enzyme of the juice is secreted in a zymogen or pro-enzyme form (typsinogen), which is activated or converted to trypsin by something contained in the mucous membrane of the small intestine (duodenum, jejunum). This something Pawlow supposes is an enzyme, and since its action is on another enzyme, "a ferment of ferments," he designated it as a kinase or enterokinase. The action of the enterokinase seems to be quite specific. According to Bayliss and Starling, tripsinogen is a stable body which cannot be changed to trypsin otherwise than by the action of the kinase; but a very small amount of the latter suffices to convert a large quantity of tripsinogen. The active trypsin itself, on the other hand, is very easily destroyed, especially in alkaline solutions. The physiological value of this very interesting relation is not clear, but it seems possible that it may serve to protect the living tissues from the powerful digestive action of the trypsin. The other enzymes of the pancreatic juice, the diastase and the lipase, are secreted apparently in active form. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE INTESTINES. 705 The Digestive Action of Pancreatic Juice. — The digestive action of the secretion depends upon the three enzymes trypsin, diastase (amylopsin), and lipase. The specific effects of each may be considered separately. Action of Trypsin. — The activated trypsinogen causes hydrolytic cleavage of the proteid molecule in a manner analogous to that described for pepsin. Its action differs from that of pepsin, however, in several respects. It attacks the proteid in neutral as well as in slightly acid or markedly alkaline solutions. Its effect upon the proteid is more rapid and powerful than that of pepsin and the proteid molecule is broken up more completely. As was said in describing the action of pepsin, it and the trypsin really act together, — the change begun by the pepsin is completed by the tryp- sin. The preliminary action of the pepsin not only hastens that of the trypsin, but to some extent alters it; a proteid submitted first to pepsin and then to trypsin is more completely broken up than if the trypsin acted alone. The steps in the hydrolysis of the proteid molecule by trypsin have been the subject of a very great amount of study, and views as to the details have changed somewhat rapidly of recent years. Kuhne supposed that the proteid molecule contains two groups, the hemi and the anti. Under the influence of the trypsin these are, on his theory, converted into corresponding proteoses, primary and secondary, and then into peptones, — hemipeptone and antipeptone. As distinguished from the pepsin, the trypsin hy- drolyzes the hemipeptone still further, splitting it up into a number of much simpler crystalline bodies, such as leucin, tyrosin, etc. Antipeptone, on the contrary, resists further hydrolysis, and among the end-products of a prolonged pancreatic digestion some peptone is always found. This view has not been supported by recent work. After a prolonged pancreatic digestion no peptone or peptone-like body can be found; in fact, no substance which gives a biuret reac- tion. Under such conditions the proteid molecule is broken up very completely into a surprising number of smaller molecules, many of which have been identified, while some have as yet escaped de- tection so far as their chemical structure is concerned. The actual products formed depend on the length of time the trypsin is allowed to act and the conditions, favorable or unfavorable, under which it acts. The end-products usually obtained most easily are tyrosin, leucin, aspartic acid, glutaminic acid, tryptophan, lysin, arginin, histidin. The first two of these substances have been known for a long time, and may be obtained easily in crystalline form from pancreatic digestions. If the trypsin is allowed to exert its complete action upon the proteid the end-products are closely similar to those obtained by boiling proteid with acids. The hydrolysis caused by the acids and by the trypsin seems to be nearly identical. The 45 706 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. numerous products obtained by this complete hydrolysis consist chiefly of amino-acids, — that is, organic acids containing one or more amido-groups (NH2) in direct union with carbon. Some of these bodies are monamino-acids, — that is, contain one NH2 group, — such as leucin, tyrosin, glycocoll, — and include substances belonging to the fatty acid series (aliphatic series), the benzene or carbocyclic series, and the heterocyclic series, such as the pyrrol and the indol group. Others are diamino-acids, containing two NH2 groups. These latter include lysin, histidin, and arginin, which, on account of their basic properties, are frequently described as nitrogenous bases, and sometimes as the hexon bases, since they contain six carbon atoms. The chemical formulas for the best known of these bodies are as follows, for their properties and chemical relationships reference must be made to the text-books on organic chemistry: I. MONAMINO-BODIES. FATTY ACID SERIES. Glycocoll or amido-acetic acid: CH2NH2COOH. This product is ob- tained in especially large quantities by hydrolysis of gelatin. Accord- ing to Abderhalden,* it is split off with difficulty by trypsin. Alanin or a-amidopropionic acid: CH3CHNH2COOH. Amidovalerianic acid: ^3^CHCHNH2COOH. Leucin or amidocaproic acid: Qg3^CHCH2CHNH2COOH. As stated above, this compound was one of the first end-products of proteid hydrolysis that was recognized. It may be obtained readily in crystalline form. CHNH2COOH Aspartic or amidosuccinic acid : CH2COOH. Glutaminicacid: CH2/g™H2COOH BENZENE OR AROMATIC SERIES. Tyrosin (para-oxyphenylamidopropionic acid) : C6H4OH . CH2 . CHNH2- COOH. This substance was also among the first recognized prod- ucts of proteid hydrolysis It occurs early in the process of pan- creatic digestion, and is easily obtained in crystalline form from the digested mixture. It is especially interesting because of the presence of a benzene nucleus, thus giving proof that the benzene grouping occurs normally in the proteid molecule. Phenylalanin (phenylamidopropionic acid) : C6H5CH2CHNH2COOH. This benzene derivative is, according to Abderhalden, split off from the proteid with difficulty by the action of trypsin, although readily produced by acid hydrolysis. PYRROL AND INDOL SERIES. CH— CH2 ! I a-Pyrrolidin carbonic acid : CH2 CHCOOH. This substance, discovered \/ NH * Abderhalden, " Zeitschrift f. physiol. Chemie," 44, 17, 1905. Consult for general description of the digestion of proteids. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE INTESTINES. 707 first by Fischer among the products of acid hydrolysis of proteids, has since been shown to occur in tryptic digestion. Like the gly- cocoll and phenylalanin, it is produced with difficulty by trypsin acting alone, but more readily if the tryptic action follows upon previous peptic digestion, as is the case in the body. C.CH3 S\ Tryptophan (skatolamido-acetic acid) : C6H4 C . CH(NH2)COOH. This \/ NH substance has long been recognized among the products of tryptic digestion by the reddish-violet color (Tiedemann and Gmelin, 1826) observed upon the addition of chlorin or bromin. Its chemical structure was determined by Hopkins and Cole (1901). According to Ellinger,* tryptophan is an indol compound of the formula C . CHCOOHCH2NH2. When fed to dogs it causes the appear- C6H40CH NH ance of kynurenic acid (C10H7NO3) in the urine. It is interesting as shpwing the existence of an indol grouping in the proteid molecule. v II. The Diamino-bodies (Hexon Bases). Lysin (a-e-diamidocaproic acid): CeHMN202 or CH2NH2(CH2)3CHNH2- COOH. Arginin (guanidin a-amidovalerianic acid) : C6HHN402 or NHCNH2NH- CH2(CH2)2CHNH2COOH. Histidin: C6H9N302. These three substances occur among the products of pancreatic digestion. They may be separated from the monamino-bodies by the fact that they are precipitated in acid solutions by phosphotungstic acid while the monamino-acids are not. The Significance of Tryptic Digestion. — It was formerly supposed that the object of peptic and tryptic digestion is to convert the insoluble and non-dialyzable proteids into the simpler, more soluble, and more diffusible peptones and proteoses. In this way absorption of proteid material was explained. This view, however, is not sufficient. On the one hand, it has not been possible to prove conclusively that peptones or proteoses are found in the blood; on the other hand, a better knowledge of the processes of tryptic or of peptic-tryptic digestion has shown that the hydrolysis does not stop at the peptone stage; the proteid molecule is split into a number of simpler, crystalline substances, the various amino- bodies. At present different views exist as to the extent of this latter process. Some believe that the proteid molecule is entirely broken down into its so-called end-products, and that in order to serve its nutritive function these products or some of them must be synthet- ically combined again during or after absorption. This view is supported, moreover, by the discovery of the existence of the enzyme erepsin (see below) in the intestinal mucosa. The action of this lat- ter enzyme is exerted especially upon the albumoses and peptones, * Ellinger, " Zeitschrift f. physiol. Chemie," 43, 325, 1904. 708 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. breaking them down into the amido-acids, so that apparently what- ever peptone or albumose may escape the final action of the trypsin before absorption is likely to be acted upon by the erepsin before reaching the blood.* Another interesting view is that suggested by Abderhalden. f According to this author, the hydrolysis of the proteid by pepsin and trypsin (and perhaps by erepsin) is not complete. Many amido-bodies, such as tyrosin, leucin, arginin, etc., are split off from the proteid molecule, but there remains behind what one may call a nucleus of the original molecule, which serves as the starting point for a synthesis. This nucleus is a substance or a number of substances intermediate between the peptone and the simpler end-products, and is spoken of as a peptid or peptoid (see appendix). Since its structure is unknown and it is probably not a simple body, it is designated as a poly peptid. Abderhalden has shown that in tryptic digestion such substances are formed. That is, substances which are not peptones, since they no longer give the biuret reaction, but which have a certain complexity of structure, since upon hydrolysis with acids they split into a number of mona- mido- and diamido-bodies. A schema of peptic-tryptic digestion from this standpoint may be given as follows : Native proteid. Peptone. Polypeptid. Tyrosin, leucin, glutaminic acid, aspartic acid, a-pyrrolidin carbonic acid, tryptophan, etc. Arginin, lysin, histidin. From either of the points of view presented it may be suggested that the value of this more or less complete splitting of the proteid of the food lies in the possibility that thereby the body is able to construct its own peculiar type of proteid. Many different kinds of proteids are taken as food and many of them if introduced directly into the blood act as foreign material incapable of nourishing the tissues. If these proteids are broken down more or less completely during digestion the tissue cells may reconstruct from the pieces a form of proteid adaptable to their needs. At present our knowledge of what takes place during the absorption of proteid is very incomplete, and a satisfactory theory of proteid nutrition is scarcely possible until this portion of the subject is cleared up. Action of the Diastatic Enzyme (Amylopsin) of the Pan- creatic Secretion. — This enzyme is found in the secretion of the * Vernon ("Journal of Physiology," 30, 330, 1904) believes that the pancreatic secretion contains two proteolytic enzymes, — trypsin proper, which converts the proteids to peptones, and pancreatic erepsin, which breaks up the peptones into the simpler end-products, the amido-bodies. t Abderhalden, loc. cit. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE INTESTINES. 709 pancreas or it may be extracted from the gland. Its action upon starchy foods is closely similar to or identical with that of ptyalin. It causes an hydrolysis of the starch with the production finally of maltose and achroodextrin. Before absorption these substances are further acted upon by the maltase of the intestinal secretion and converted to dextrose. The starchy food that escapes digestion in the mouth and stomach becomes mixed with this enzyme in the duodenum, and from that time until it reaches the end of the large intestine conditions are favorable for its conversion to maltose and dextrin. Most of this digestion is probably completed, under normal conditions, before the contents of the intestinal canal reach the ileo- cecal valve. Action of the Lipolytic Enzyme (Lipase, Steapsin). — The importance of the pancreatic secretion in the digestion of fats was first clearly stated by Bernard (1849). We know now that this secre- tion contains an active enzyme capable of hydrolyzing or saponifying the neutral fats. These latter bodies are chemically esters of the trihydric alcohol glycerin. When hydrolyzed they break up into glycerin and the constituent fatty acid. The action of lipase may be represented, therefore, by the following reaction, in the case of palmitin : C3H5(C15H31COO)3 + 3H20 = C3H5(OH)3 + 3(C15H31COOH) Palmitin. Glycerin. Palmitic acid. When lipase from any source is added to neutral oils its splitting action is readily recognized by the development of an acid reaction due to the formation of the fatty acid. If a bit of fresh pancreas is added to butter, for example, and the mixture is kept at the body temperature the hydrolysis of the fats is soon made evident by the rancid odor due to the butyric acid produced. When pancreatic juice is mixed with oils or liquid fats two phenomena may be noticed: first, the splitting of the fat already referred to, and, second, the emulsification of the fat. The latter process is very striking. An oil is emulsified when it is broken up into minute globules that do not coalesce. Artificial emulsions may be made by vigorous and prolonged shaking of the oil in a viscous solution of soap, mucilage, etc. Milk may be regarded as a natural emulsion that separates slowly on standing, as the fat rises to the top to form the cream. When a little pancreatic juice is added to oil at the body temperature the mixture, after standing for some time, will emulsify readily with very little shaking or even spontaneously. It is now known* that the emulsification is due to the formation of soaps. The lipase splits some of the fats, and the fatty acid liberated combines with the alkaline salts present to form soaps. The emulsification produced * See Ratchford, " Journal of Physiology," 12, 27, 1891. 710 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. under these conditions is very fine and quite permanent, and it was formerly believed that the formation of this emulsion is the main function of the pancreatic juice so far as fats are concerned. It was thought that in the form of fine droplets the fat may be taken up directly by the epithelial cells of the villi, and this view was supported by the histological fact that during the digestion of fats the epithelial cells may be shown to contain fine oil drops in their interior. The tendency of recent work, however, has been to indicate that the fats are completely split into fatty acids and glycerin before absorption, and that the emulsification may be regarded, from a physiological standpoint, as a mechanical preparation for the action of the lipase rather than as a direct preparation for the act of absorption. The two products of the action of the lipase, the glycerin and the fatty acid, are absorbed by the epithelium and again combined to form neutral fat. In connection with this fact of a synthesis of the split products to form neutral fat, the discovery by Kastle and Loeven- hart (see p. 660) that the action of lipase is reversible assumes much significance. It seems quite possible that the same enzyme may cause both the splitting of the fat and the synthesis of the split products, not only in the intestine during absorption, but in the various tissues during the metabolism or the storage of fat. Lipase is found in the blood and in many tissues, — muscle, liver, mammary gland,* etc., — and during its nutritive history in the body the fat may be split and synthesized a number of times. In this connection it is interesting to note that the process of splitting does not involve much work. Very little heat is liberated in the process, and a corre- spondingly small amount of energy is needed for the synthesis. f The lipase as formed in the pancreas is easily destroyed, especially by acids. For this reason probably it is not found usually in simple extracts of the gland made by laboratory methods. It should be added, also, that the action of this enzyme is aided very materially by the presence of bile. This latter secretion contains no lipase itself, but mixtures of bile and pancreatic juice split the neutral fats much more rapidly than the pancreatic juice alone. The Intestinal Secretion (Succus Entericus) .— The small intestine is lined with tubular glands, the crypts of Lieberkuhn, which in parts of the intestine at least give rise to a liquid secretion, the so-called intestinal juice. To obtain this secretion recourse has been had to the operation known as the Thiry-Vella fistula. In this operation a given portion of the intestine is separated from the remainder without injuring its blood-vessels or nerves and the two ends are sutured into the abdominal wall. In the loop thus isolated the secretions may be collected and experiments may be made upon * See Loevenhart, " Amer. Journal of Physiology," 6, 331, 1902. t Consult Herzog, " Zeitschrift f. physiol. Chemie, V 37, 383, 1903. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE INTESTINES. 711 the digestion and absorption of various substances. The secretion from these loops is usually said to be small in quantity, especially in the jejunum. Pregl estimates that as much as three liters may be formed in the whole of the small intestine in the course of a day, but this estimate does not rest upon very satisfactory data. The liquid is distinctly alkaline, owing to the presence of sodium carbonate. Experiments have shown that this liquid has little or no digestive action except upon the starches, and it may perhaps be doubted whether it is a true secretion. Extracts of the walls of the small intestine or the juice squeezed from these walls have been found to contain four or five different enzymes and to exert a most important influence upon intestinal digestion. Whether these enzymes are actually secreted into the lumen of the intestine is not satisfactorily shown, but since they are contained in the intestinal wall we must regard them as secretory products and consider them as the impor- tant and characteristic feature of the intestinal secretion. These enzymes and their actions are as follows : 1 . Enterokinase (see p. 704) , an enzyme which in some way activates the proteolytic enzyme of the pancreatic juice, by converting the tryp- sinogen to trypsin. 2. Erepsin. This enzyme, discovered by Cohnheim,* acts especially upon the deutero-albumoses and peptones, causing further hydrolysis. Whether its splitting action upon the peptones is complete is not as yet known, but, as was said above (p. 705), the natural suggestion regarding this enzyme is that it supplements the work begun by the trypsin. 3. Inverting enzymes capable of converting the disaccharids into the monosaccharids. These enzymes are three in number: maltase, which acts upon maltose (and dextrin) ; invertase or invertin, which acts upon cane-sugar; and lactase, which acts upon lactose. The maltase acts upon the products formed in the digestion of starches, the maltose and dextrin, converting them to dextrose according to the general formula: C12H22Ou + H20 = C6H1206 + C6Hi206 Maltose. Dextrose. Dextrose. In the same way invertase converts cane-sugar to dextrose and levu- lose, and lactase changes milk-sugar to dextrose and galactose. This inverting action is necessary to prepare the carbohydrate food for nutritive purposes. Double sugars can not be used by the tissues and would escape in the urine, but in the form of dextrose or dextrose and levulose they are readily used by the tissues in their normal metabolic processes. 4. Lastly, the substance secretin, which, as explained above, may play such an important role in the control of the secretion of the pan- creas, is formed in the walls of the small intestine. It is not an enzyme, but a more stable and definite chemical substance which is secreted or formed in the intestinal mucosa in a preliminary form, prosecretin, and under the influence of acids is changed to secretin. In this latter form it is absorbed, carried to the pancreas, and causes a flow of pancreatic secretion. * Cohnheim, " Zeitschrift f . physiol. Chemie," 33, 451, 1901; also 35, 134 et seq. 712 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. Absorption in the Small Intestine. — Absorption takes place very readily in the small intestine. The general correctness of this statement may be shown by the use of isolated loops of the intestine. Salt solutions of varying strengths or even blood-serum nearly identical in composition with the animals' own blood may be ab- sorbed completely from these loops. Examination of the contents of the intestine in the duodenum and at the ileocecal valve shows that the products formed in digestion have largely disappeared in traversing this distance. All the information that we possess in- dicates, in fact, that the mucous membrane of the small intestine absorbs readily, and it is one of the problems of this part of physiology to explain the means by which this absorption is effected. Anatomi- cally two paths are open to the products absorbed. They may enter the blood directly by passing into the capillaries of the villi, or they may enter the lacteals of the villi, pass into the lymph circulation, and through the thoracic duct of the lymphatic system eventually reach the blood vascular system. The older physiologists assumed that absorption takes place exclusively through the central lacteals of the villi, and hence these vessels were described as the absorbents. We now know that the digested and resynthesized fats are absorbed by way of the lacteals, but that the other products of digestion are absorbed mainly through the blood-vessels and therefore enter the portal system and pass through the liver before reaching the general circulation. According to observations made upon a patient with a fistula at the end of the small intestine,* food begins to pass into the large intestine in from two to five and a quarter hours after eating, and it requires from nine to twenty-three hours before the last of a meal has passed the ileocecal valve; this estimate includes, of course, the time in the stomach. During this passage absorption of the digested products takes place nearly completely. In the fistula case referred to above it was found that 85 per cent, of the proteid had disappeared, and similar facts are known regarding the other food- stuffs. The problems that have excited the greatest interest have been, first, the exact form in which the digested products are ab- sorbed, and, second, the means by which this absorption is effected. With regard to the last question, much work has been done to ascertain whether the known physical laws of diffusion, osmosis, and imbibition are sufficient to account for the movements of the absorbed substances or whether it is necessary to refer them in part to some unknown activities of the living epithelial cells. It would seem that diffusion and osmosis occur in the intestines. Concentrated solutions of neutral salts, — sodium chlorid, for instance, — if introduced into a Thiry-Vella loop, cause a flow of water into * Macfadyen, Nencki, and Sieber, "Archiv f. experiment. Pathol, u. Pharmakol.," 28, 311, 1891. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE INTESTINES. 713 the lumen in accordance with their high osmotic pressure, and, on the other hand, some of the sodium chlorid diffuses into the blood in accordance with the laws of diffusion. It seems equally clear, however, that absorption as it actually takes place is not governed simply by known physical laws. Thus, the animal's own serum,* possessing presumably the same concentration and osmotic pres- sure as the animal's blood, is absorbed completely from a Thiry- Vella loop. So also it has been shown that in the absorption of salts from the intestine f the rapidity of absorption stands in no direct relation to the diffusion velocity. . The energy that controls absorption resides, therefore, in the wall of the intestine, presumably in the epithelial cells, and constitutes a special form of imbibition which is not yet understood. That this particular form of energy is connected with the living structure is shown by the fact that when the walls are injured by the action of sodium fluorid, potas- sium arsenate, etc., their absorptive power is diminished and absorption then follows the laws of diffusion and osmosis. J Absorption of the Carbohydrates. — Our carbohydrate food is absorbed, for the most part, as simple sugars, — monosaccharids. As has been said, there is reason to believe that but little sugar is absorbed in the stomach. Cane-sugar and milk-sugar are inverted in the small intestine by invertase and lactase, the first being con- verted to dextrose and levulose, the second to dextrose and galactose. If, however, these substances are fed in excess they are absorbed in part without conversion to simple sugar, and in that case may be eliminated in the urine. The bulk of our carbohydrate food is taken, however, in the form of starch, and the conditions for absorption in this case are more favorable. The time required for the digestion of the starch to maltose and dextrin, and the subsequent inversion of these substances to dextrose, insures a slower and more complete absorption. Five hundred grams or more of starch may be digested and absorbed in the course of the day and it all reaches the blood in the form of dextrose. This dextrose enters the portal vein and is distributed first to the liver. In this organ the excess of sugar is withdrawn from the blood and stored as glycogen, so that the amount of sugar in the general circulation is thereby kept quite constant, — about 0.15 per cent. When a large amount of carbohydrate food is eaten, however, it is possible that the liver may not be able to remove the excess completely. In that case the amount of sugar in the gen- eral circulation may be increased above normal, giving a condition of hyperglycemia. In such cases the excess is likely to be excreted * Heidenhain, "Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 56, 579, 1894. t Wallace and Cushny, " Archiv f . die gesammte Physiologie, " 77, 202, 1899. JCohnheim, "Zeitschrift f. Biologie," 37, 443, 1899. 714 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. in the urine, giving the phenomenon known as "alimentary glyco- suria." The amount of any carbohydrate that can be eaten, without producing a condition of alimentary glycosuria is designated by Hofmeister* as the assimilation limit of that carbohydrate. If taken beyond this limit it forms a physiological excess, and some is lost in the urine. The assimilation limit varies with a great many conditions; but, so far as the different forms of carbohydrates are concerned, it is lowest for the milk-sugar and highest for starch. The simple sugars dialyze easily, and it would be natural to suppose that they are absorbed into the blood by a simple process of diffusion. Experimental facts, however, do not support this view entirely. It is stated that the absorption of sugars does not vary directly as their velocity of diffusion, and in this case, as with the other products of digestion, it is necessary to assume that work is done by the wall of the intestine itself, probably the epithelial cells. So far as the carbohydrates escape absorption as sugar they are liable to undergo acid fermentation from the bacteria always present in the intestine. As the result of this fermentation there may be produced acetic acid, lactic acid, butyric acid, succinic acid, carbon dioxid, alcohol, hydrogen, etc. This fermentation probably occurs to some extent in the small intestines under normal conditions. Macfadyen,f in the case already referred to, found that the contents of the intestine at the ileocecal valve contained acid equivalent to that of a 0.1 per cent, solution of acetic acid. Under less normal conditions, such as excess of sugars in the diet or deficient absorp- tion, the large production of acids may lead to irritation of the intes- tines,— diarrhea, etc. Absorption of Fats. — Numerous theories have been held in regard to the mode of absorption of fats. It has been supposed that the emulsified (neutral) fat is ingested directly by the epithelial cells, that the fat droplets enter between the epithelial cells in the so-called cement substance, that the fat droplets are ingested by leucocytes that lie between the epithelial cells, or lastly that the fat is first split into fatty acid and glycerin and is absorbed by the epithelial cells in these forms. The tendency of recent work is to favor this last view. During digestion the epithelial cells contain fat droplets without doubt, but it seems probable that these droplets are formed in situ by a synthesis of the absorbed glycerin and fatty acids. The border of the cell is said to be free from fat globules, — a fact which would indicate that the neutral fat is not mechanically ingested as oil drops. But, granting that the fat is absorbed in solution, as fatty acids and glycerin, the mechanism of absorption remains unexplained. It is * Hofmeister, "Archiv f. exper. Pathol, u. Pharmakol.," 25, 240, 1889, and 26, 355, 1890. t Macfadyen, Nencki, and Sieber, loc, cit. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE INTESTINES. 715 known that the bile as well as the pancreatic juice plays an important part in the process. The pancreatic juice furnishes the lipase, the bile furnishes the bile salts (glycocholate and taurocholate of sodium) which aid the lipase in splitting the neutral fat, and moreover aid greatly the absorption of the split fats. This latter function is due probably to the fact that the bile (bile salts) dissolves the fatty acids readily* and thus brings them into contact, in soluble form, with the epithelial cells. When the bile is drained off from the intestine by a fistula of the gall-bladder or duct, a large proportion of the fatty foods escapes absorption and appears in the feces. Direct observation shows that the fat after passing the epithelial lin- ing and entering the stroma of the villus is taken up by the lymphatic vessels, the so-called lacteals. This fact is beautifully demonstrated by the mere appearance of the lymphatics of the mesentery after a meal containing fats. These vessels are injected with milky chyle during the period of absorption so that their entire course is revealed. The chyle oh microscopical exami- nation is found to contain fat in the form of an extremely fine emulsion. In this form it is carried to the thoracic duct and thence to the venous circulation. For hours after a meal the blood contains this chyle fat. If a specimen of blood is taken during this time and centrifugalized in the usual way, the chyle fat may be collected at the top in the form of a cream. It is an easy matter to insert a cannula into the thoracic duct at the point at which it opens into the subclavian and jugular veins and thus collect the entire amount of fat absorbed from the intestines by way of the lacteals. Experiments of this kind show that, after deducting the amount of fat that escapes absorption and is lost in the feces, the amount that may be recovered from the thoracic duct is less than that taken in the food. It seems probable, therefore, that some of the fat is absorbed directly by the blood-vessels of the villi, probably in the form of fatty acids or soaps. The portion thus absorbed enters the portal vein and passes through the liver before reaching the general circulation. The liver holds back more or less of the fat taking this route, as it is found that during absorption the liver cells show an accumulation of fat droplets in their interior.f The amount of fat that may be absorbed from the intestines varies with the nature of the fat. Experiments show that the more fluid fats, such as olive oil, are absorbed more com- pletely,— that is, less is lost in the feces than in the case of the more solid fats. Comparative experiments have given such results as the following: Olive oil, — absorption, 97.7 per cent.; goose and pork fat, 97.5 per cent.; mutton fat, 90 to 92.5 percent.; spermaceti, 15 per *See Moore and Rockwood, "Journal of Physiology," 21, 58, 1897; also Moore and Parker, "Proceedings, Royal Society," London, 58, 64, 1901. t See Frank, " Archiv f. Physiologie, " 1892, 497, and 1894, 297. 716 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. cent. The amount of fat that may be lost in the feces varies also with other conditions. If, for instance, an excess is taken with the food or if the bile flow is diminished or suppressed the percentage in the feces is increased. The usual amount of fat allowed as a maximum in dietaries is from 100 to 120 gms. daily. Absorption of Proteids. — Most of the experimental work on record shows that the digested proteids are absorbed by the blood- vessels of the villi, although after excessive feeding of proteid a portion may be taken up also in the lymphatics.*. This accepted belief rests upon two facts: First (Schmidt-Mulheim), if the thoracic duct (and right lymphatic duct) is ligated, so as to shut off the lym- phatic circulation, an animal will absorb and metabolize the usual amount of proteid, as is indicated by the urea excreted during the period. Second (Munk), if a fistula of the thoracic duct is established and the total lymph flow from the intestines is collected during the period of absorption after a diet of proteid, it is found that there is no increase in the quantity of the lymph or in its proteid contents. The form in which proteid is absorbed remains, however, a mystery. Under normal conditions the proteid food is digested by the success- ive actions of pepsin, trypsin, and probably erepsin. During this digestion peptones and proteoses are formed and may be absorbed as such, or they may be further broken down by trypsin and erepsin to the amido-bodies, leucin, tyrosin, arginin, etc., and the intermediate compounds, the polypeptids or peptoids (see p. 708), and be absorbed in the form of these split products. Examination of the blood does not show, however, the presence of any of these bodies, and in spite of the fact that the process of absorption is long continued and the total amount of absorbed products in any given specimen of blood may therefore be very small, it is perplexing not to be able to ob- tain indubitable proof of the existence in the blood of some of the products of digestion. Several possibilities have been suggested. It is conceivable that the peptones or the more simple split products may be synthesized in the wall of the intestine or in the liver to the proteids of the blood, the serum-albumin or globulin; it is possi- ble that many of the end-products of the digestive splitting may be further oxidized and converted to urea in the liver and only a fractional part be really synthesized into the proteids of the body, or it is possible that the absorbed proteid exists in the blood in some special form not as yet recognized. The whole question is evidently one that can not be discussed very profitably at present ; it awaits the results of further investigation. In this connection attention should be directed to the fact that many forms of proteid may be absorbed apparently without previous digestion. This fact has been demonstrated for isolated loops of the small intestine and * See Mendel, " American Journal of Physiology, " 2, 137, 1899. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE INTESTINES. 717 also for parts of the large intestine. It is, moreover, borne out by the medical practice of giving enemata into the rectum when the conditions are such that the patient can not be fed in the normal way. That absorption and utilization of the proteid take place under such conditions is shown not only by the improved nutritive condition of the individual, but also by the increased output of nitrogen in the urine. This phenomenon occurs in parts of the intestinal canal in which normally no proteolytic enzymes occur, so that the whole process must be referred to an activity of the cells composing the walls of the intestine. There seems at present little grounds for a satisfactory explanation of the absorption of proteids, with or without digestion, by a direct application of the known laws of osmosis, diffusion, and imbibition. Examination of the contents of the small intestine at its junction with the large shows that under normal conditions most of the proteid has been ab- sorbed before reaching this point. The process is continued in the large intestine, modified somewhat by bacterial action, and the amount that finally escapes absorption and appears in the feces varies, in perfectly normal individuals, with the character of the proteid eaten. According to Munk,* the easily digestible animal foods — such as milk, eggs, and meat — are absorbed to the extent of 97 to 99 per cent., while with vegetable foods the utilization is less complete; from 17 to 30 per cent, of the proteid may be lost in the feces if the vegetable food is in such form as not to be attacked readily by the digestive secretions. Digestion and Absorption in the Large Intestine. — Observa- tions upon the secretions of the large intestine have been made upon human beings in cases of anus praeternaturalis, in which the lower portion of the intestine was practically isolated, and also upon lower animals, in which an artificial anus was established at the end of the small intestine. These observations all indicate that the secretion of the large intestine, while it contains much mucus and shows an alkaline reaction, is not characterized by the presence of distinctive enzymes. When the contents of the small intestine pass the valve they still contain a certain amount of unabsorbed food material. As was stated in the chapter on the movements of the intestine, this material remains a long time in the large intestine, and since it contains the digestive enzymes received in the duodenum the digestive and absorptive processes no doubt continue as in the small intestine. This general fact is well illustrated in experiments made upon dogs most of whose small intestine (70 to 83 per cent.) had been removed.f These animals could digest and absorb well, and formed * See Munk, " Ergebnisse der Physiologie, " vol. i, part I, 1902, article, " Resorption," for literature and discussion. t Erlanger and Hewlett, "American Journal of Physiology," 6, 1, 1902. 718 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. normal feces, provided care was taken with the diet. An excess of fat or indigestible material caused diarrhea and serious loss of food material in the feces. An interesting feature in the large intestine is the marked absorption of water. In the small intestine no doubt water is absorbed in large quantities, but its loss is evidently made good by osmosis or secretion of water into the intestine, since the contents at the ileocecal valve are quite as fluid as at the pylorus. In the large intestine the absorption of water is not compensated by a secretion; the material becomes more and more solid as it ap- proaches the rectum, and is thus formed into the feces. The alkaline reaction of the contents of the large intestine makes a favorable environment for the growth of bacteria, particularly the putrefactive bacteria that attack proteid material. Putrefaction is a normal occurrence in the large intestine, and much interest has been shown in its extent and its possible physiological significance. Bacterial Action in the Small Intestine. — Bacteria are con- stantly present in both the large and the small intestine. Under normal conditions, however, it would seem that in the small intestine only those bacteria capable of fermenting carbohydrate food show any distinct activity. Putrefactive fermentation of proteid material is limited or absent in this part of the intestine as long as the products of proteid digestion are promptly absorbed. Conditions that prevent or retard this absorption favor the occurrence of proteid putrefaction. Opinions among investigators differ as to the means by which the proteid contents are protected from the action of the bacteria. It has been shown that the presence of carbohydrate material has a restraining effect upon proteid putrefaction. The simplest explana- tion of this relation is that the fermentation of the carbohydrates gives rise to a number of organic acids, — lactic, acetic, etc., — and these acids inhibit the action of the proteid bacteria. To make this explanation satisfactory, however, it is necessary to show that the contents of the small intestine possess an acid reaction. Concerning this point, however, opinions also differ. The secretions of the small intestine are all alkaline and we should expect their contents to have this reaction. Examination shows that the contents of the small intestine are acid or not according to the indicator used. With phenolphthalein they may give an acid reaction, while with litmus, lakmoid, etc., no such reaction is obtained.* Such a result as this indicates that no strong organic acids, such as acetic and lactic, are present, the phenolphthalein being affected possibly by the C02. As Munk has stated, however, it seems that the contents of the small intestine throughout the duodenum and jejunum are at least never * Consult Macfadyen, Nencki, and Sieber, loc. cit.; Moore and Bergin, "American Journal of Physiology," 3, 316, 1900; Munk, " Centralblatt f. Physiologie," 16, 33 and 146, 1902. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE INTESTINES. 719 alkaline, and when carbohydrates are used the reaction may not only be acid to phenolphthalein, but also to the stronger indicators. On the whole, therefore, it would seem probable that the small amount or total lack of proteid putrefaction in the small intestine is due in part to the rapid absorption of the digested proteid and in part to an unfavorable reaction. Some observers contend that there is a struggle for existence or antagonism between the bacteria acting upon carbohydrates and those living upon proteids. When the former have conditions favorable for growth, their increase in some way affects injuriously the proteid bacteria.* Bacterial Action in the Large Intestine. — In the large intestine proteid putrefaction is a constant and normal occurrence. The reaction here is stated to be alkaline, and whatever proteid may have escaped digestion and absorption is in turn acted upon by the bacteria and undergoes so-called putrefactive fermentation. The splitting up of the proteid molecule by this process is very complete, and differs in some of its products from the results of hydrolytic cleavage as caused by acids or by trypsin. The list of end-products of putre- faction is a long one. Besides peptones, proteoses, ammonia, and the various amido-acids, there may be produced such substances as indol, skatol, phenol, phenylpropionic and phenylacetic acids, fatty acids, carbon dioxid, hydrogen, marsh gas, hydrogen sulphid, etc. Many of these products are given off in the feces, while others are absorbed in part and excreted subsequently in the urine. In this latter connection especial interest attaches to the phenol, indol, and skatol. Phenol or carbolic acid, C6H5OH, after absorption is com- bined with sulphuric acid, to form an ethereal sulphate (conjugated sulphate) or phenolsulphonic acid, C6H5OS02OH, and in this form is found in the urine. So also with cresol. The indol, C8H7N, and skatol (methyl-indol), C9H9N, are also absorbed, undergo oxidation to indoxyl and skatoxyl, and are then combined or conjugated with sulphuric acid, like the phenol, and in this form are found in the urine — C8H6NOS02OH, or indoxyl-sulphuric acid, and C9H8NOS02OH, skatoxyl-sulphuric acid. These bodies have long been known to occur in the urine, and the proof that they arise primarily from putre- faction of proteid material in the large intestine is so conclusive as not to admit of any doubt. The amount to which they occur in the urine is, therefore, an indication of the extent of putrefaction in the large intestine. Is the Putrefactive Process of Physiological Importance? — Recognizing that fermentation by means of bacteria is a normal occurrence in the gastro-intestinal canal, the question has arisen whether this process is in any way necessary to normal digestion and nutrition. It is well known that excessive bacterial action may lead * See Bienstock, " Archiv f. Hygiene," 39, 390, 1901. 720 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. to intestinal troubles, such as diarrhea, or to more serious interference with general nutrition owing to the formation of toxins. It is, however, possible that some amount of bacterial action may be necessary for completely normal digestion. As a special case it has been pointed out that the gastro-intestinal tract is not provided with enzymes capable of acting upon cellulose, a material that forms such an important constituent of vegetable foods. Bacteria, on the other hand, may hydrolyze the cellulose and render it useful in nutrition. Leaving aside this special case, the question as to the necessity of bacterial action has been investigated directly by attempting to rear young animals under perfectly sterile conditions. Nuttall and Thierf elder * report some very interesting experiments upon guinea pigs in which the young animals from birth were kept sterile and fed with perfectly sterile food. They found that the animals lived and increased in weight, and concluded therefore that the intestinal bacteria are not necessary to normal nutrition. This conclusion is supported by the observations of Levin,| who finds that animals in the Arctic regions in many cases have no bacteria in their intestines. Schottelius J reports contrary results upon chickens. When kept sterile they lost steadily in weight and showed normal growth only when supplied with food containing bacteria. We may conclude, however, that the evidence at present indicates that the bacterial fermentation is not essential, although under the actual conditions of life it plays a part in the digestive history of the food. Composition of the Feces. — The feces differ widely in amount and in composition with the character of the food. Upon a diet composed exclusively of meats they are small in amount and dark in color; with an ordinary mixed diet the amount is increased; and it is largest with an exclusively vegetable diet, especially with vege- tables containing a large amount of indigestible material. The average weight of the feces in twenty-four hours upon a mixed diet is given as 170 gms., while with a vegetable diet it may amount to as much as 400 or 500 gms. The quantitative composition, therefore, varies greatly with the diet. Qualitatively, we find in the feces the following things: (1) Indigestible material, such as ligaments of meat or cellulose from vegetables. (2) Undigested material, such as fragments of meat, starch, or fats which have in some way escaped digestion. Naturally, the quantity of this material present is slight under normal conditions. Some fats, however, are almost always found in feces, either as neutral fats or as fatty acids, and to a small extent as calcium or magnesium soaps. The quantity of fat found is * Nuttall and Thierfelder, " Zeitschrift f. physiol. Chemie," 21, 109, 1895; 22, 62, 1896; 23, 231, 1897. t " Skandinavisches Archiv f. Physiologie," 16, 249, 1904. j " Archiv f. Hygiene," 42, 48, 1902. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE INTESTINES. 721 increased by an increase of the fats in the food or by a deficient secretion of bile. (3) Products of the intestinal secretions. Evi- dence has accumulated in recent years* to show that the feces in man on an average diet are composed in part of the material of the intestinal secretion. The nitrogen of the feces, formerly supposed to represent only undigested food, seems rather to have its origin largely in these secretions, and, therefore, like the nitrogen of the urine, represents so much metabolism in the body. (4) Products of bac- terial decomposition. The most characteristic of these products are indol and skatol. They are crystalline bodies possessing a disagree- able, fecal odor; this is especially true of skatol, to which the odor of the feces is mainly due. (5) Cholesterin, which is found always in small amounts, and is probably derived from the bile. (6) Ex- cretin, a crystallizable, non-nitrogenous substance to which the formula C78H156S02has been assigned, is found in minute quantities. (7) Mucus and epithelial cells thrown off from the intestinal wall. (8) Pigment. In addition to the color due to the undigested food or to the metallic compounds contained in it, there is normally present in the feces a pigment, urobilin or stercobilin, derived from the pigments (bilirubin) of the bile. Urobilin is formed from the bilirubin by reduction in the large intestine. (9) Inorganic salts — ■ salts of sodium, potassium, calcium, magnesium, and iron. The significance of the calcium and iron salts will be referred to in a subse- quent chapter, when speaking of their nutritive importance. (10) micro-organisms. Great quantities of bacteria of different kinds are found in the feces. In addition to the feces, there is found often in the large intestine a quantity of gas that may also be eliminated through the rectum. This gas varies in composition. The following substances have been found at one time or another: CH4, C02, H, N, H2S. They arise mainly from the bacterial fermentation of the proteids, although some of the N may be derived from air swallowed with the food. * Prausnitz, " Zeitschrift f. Biologie," 35, 335, 1897; and Tsuboi, ibid., p. 68. 46 V CHAPTER XLIV. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE LIVER AND THE SPLEEN. The liver plays an important part in the general nutrition of the body. Its functions are manifold, but in the long run they depend upon the properties of the liver cell, which constitutes the anatomical and physiological unit of the organ. These cells are seemingly uniform in structure throughout the whole substance of the liver, but to understand clearly the different functions they fulfill one must have a clear idea of their anatomical relations to one another and to the blood-vessels, the lymphatics, and the bile-ducts. The histol- ogy of the liver lobule, and the relationship of the portal vein, the hepatic artery, and the bile-duct to the lobule, must be obtained from the text-books upon histology and anatomy. It is sufficient here to recall the fact that each lobule is supplied with blood coming in part from the portal vein and in part from the hepatic artery. The blood from the former source contains the soluble products absorbed from the alimentary canal, such as sugar and proteid, and these absorbed products are submitted to the metabolic activity of the liver cells before reaching the general circulation. The hepatic artery brings to the liver cells the arterialized blood sent out to the systemic circu- lation from the left ventricle. In addition, each lobule gives origin to the bile capillaries which arise between the separate cells and which carry off the bile formed within the cells. In accordance with these facts, the physiology of the liver cell falls naturally into two parts, — one treating of the formation, composition, and physiological signifi- cance of bile, and the other dealing with the metabolic changes pro- duced in the mixed blood of the portal vein and the hepatic artery as it flows through the lobules. In this latter division the main phenomena to be studied are the formation of urea and the formation and significance of glycogen. Bile. — From a physiological standpoint, bile is partly an excre- tion carrying off certain waste products, and partly a digestive secre- tion playing an important role in the absorption of fats, and possibly in other ways. Bile is a continuous secretion, but in animals possess- ing a gall-bladder its ejection into the duodenum is intermittent. Bile is easily obtained from living animals by establishing a fistula of the bile-duct or, as seems preferable, of the gall-bladder. The latter operation has been performed a number of times on human beings. In some cases the entire supply of bile has been diverted in 722 ubrXf PHYSIOLOGY OF THE LIVER AND SPLEEW^/,,/. 723 this way to the exterior, and it is an interesting physiological that such patients may continue to enjoy fair health, showing that, whatever part the bile takes normally in digestion and absorption, its passage into the intestine is not absolutely necessary to the nu- trition of the body. The quantity of bile secreted during the day has been estimated for human beings of average weight (43 to 73 kgms.) as varying between 500 and 800 c.c. This estimate is based upon observations on cases of biliary fistula.* Chemical analyses of the bile show that, in addition to the water and salts, it contains bile pigments, bile acids, cholesterin, lecithin, neutral fats and soaps, sometimes a trace of urea, and a mucilaginous nucleo-albumin for- merly designated improperly as mucin. The last-mentioned sub- stance is not formed in the liver cells, but is added to the bile by the mucous membrane of the bile-ducts and gall-bladder. The quantity of these substances present in the bile varies in different animals and under different conditions. As an illustration of their relative importance in human bile and of the limits of variation, the two following analyses by Hammarstenf may be quoted: i. ii. Solids 2.520 2.840 Water 97.480 97.160 Mucin and pigment 0.529 0.910 Bile salts 0.931 0.814 Taurocholate 0.3034 0.053 Glycocholate 0.6276 0.761 Fatty acids from soap 0.1230 0.024 Cholesterin 0.0630 0.096 ^fhin}... 0.0220 0.1286 Soluble salts 0.8070 0.8051 Insoluble salts 0.0250 0.0411 The color of bile varies in different animals according to the pre- ponderance of one or the other of the main bile pigments, bilirubin and biliverdin. The bile of carnivorous animals has usually a golden color, owing to the presence of bilirubin, while that of the her- bivora is a bright green from the biliverdin. The color of human bile seems to vary : according to some authorities, it is yellow or golden yellow, and this seems especially true of the bile as found in the gall- bladder of the cadaver; according to others, it is of a dark-olive color with the greenish tint predominating. Its reaction is feebly alkaline, and its specific gravity varies in human bile from 1.050 or 1.040 to 1.010. Human bile does not give a distinctive absorption spectrum, but the bile of some herbivora, after exposure to the air at least, gives a characteristic spectrum. • *Copeman and Winston, "Journal of Physiology," 10, 213, 1889; Rob- son, "Proceedings of the Royal Society," London, 47, 499, 1890; Pfaff and Balch, "Journal of Experimental Medicine," 2, 49, 1897. f Reported in " Centralblatt f. Physiologie," 1894, No. 8. 724 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. Bile Pigments. — Bile, according to the animal from which it is obtained, contains one or the other, or a mixture, of the two pigments bilirubin and biliverdin. Biliverdin is supposed to stand to bilirubin in the relation of an oxidation product. Bilirubin is given the formula C16H18N203, and biliverdin C16H18N204, the latter being prepared readily from pure specimens of the former by oxidation. These pigments give a characteristic reaction, known as" Gmelin's reaction," with nitric acid containing some nitrous acid (nitric acid with a yellow color). If a drop of bile and a drop of nitric acid are brought into contact, the former undergoes a succession of color changes, the order being green, blue, violet, red, and reddish yellow. The play of colors is due to successive oxidations of the bile pigments; starting with bilirubin, the first stage (green) is due to the formation of bili- verdin. The pigments formed in some of the other stages have been isolated and named. The reaction is very delicate, and it is often used to detect the presence of bile pigments in other liquids — urine, for example. The bile pigments originate from hemoglobin. This origin was first indicated by the fact that in old blood clots or in extravasations there was found a crystalline product, the so-called " hematoidin," which was undoubtedly derived from hemoglobin, and which upon more careful examination was proved to be identical with bilirubin. This origin, which has since been made probable by other reactions, is now universally accepted. It is supposed that when the blood-corpuscles disintegrate the hemoglobin is brought to the liver, and there, under the influence of the liver cells, is converted to an iron-free compound, bilirubin or biliverdin. It is very signifi- cant that the iron separated by this means from the hemoglobin is, for the most part, retained in the liver, a small portion only being secreted in the bile. It seems probable that the iron held back in the liver is again used in some way to make new hemoglobin in the hematopoietic organs. The bile pigments are carried in the bile to the duodenum and are mixed with the food in its long passage through the intestine. Under normal conditions neither bilirubin nor bili- verdin occurs in the feces, but in their place is found a reduction prod- uct, urobilin or stercobilin, formed in the large intestine. More- over, it is believed that some of the bile pigment is reabsorbed as it passes along the intestine, is carried to the liver in the portal blood, and is again eliminated. That this action occurs, or may occur, has been made probable by experiments of Wertheimer* on dogs. It happens that sheep's bile contains a pigment (cholohematin) that gives a characteristic spectrum. If some of this pigment is injected into the mesenteric veins of a dog it is eliminated while passing through the liver, and can be recognized unchanged in the bile. * "Archives de physiologie normale et pathologique," 1892, p. 577. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE LIVER AND SPLEEN. 725 The value of this " circulation of the bile/' so far as the pigments are concerned, is not apparent. Bile Acids. — " Bile acids" is the name given to two organic acids, glycocholic and taurocholic, which are always present in bile, and, indeed, form very important constituents of that secretion; they occur in the form of their respective sodium salts. In human bile both acids are usually found, but the proportion of taurocholate is variable, and in some cases it may be absent altogether. Among herbivora the glycocholate predominates, as a rule, although there are some exceptions; among the carnivora, on the other hand, taurocholate occurs usually in greater quantities, and in the dog's bile it is present alone. Glycocholic acid has the formula C26H43N06, and taurocholic acid the formula C26H45NS07. Each of them can be obtained in the form of crystals. When boiled with acids or alka- lies these acids take up water and undergo hydrolytic cleavage, the reaction being represented by the following equations: C^H^NO, + H20 = C^H^O, + CH?(NH2)COOH. Glycocholic acid. Cholic acid. Glycocoll (amido-acetic-acid). C„H„NS07 + H20 = C24H40O5 + C2H4NH2S02OH Taurocholic acid. Cholic acid. Taurin (amido-ethyl- sul phonic acid). These reactions are interesting not only in that they throw light on the structure of the acids, but also because similar reactions doubtless take place in the intestine, cholic acid having been detected in the intestinal contents. As the formulas show, cholic acid is formed in the decomposition of each acid, and we may regard the bile acids as compounds produced by the synthetic union of cholic acid with glycocoll in the one case and with taurin in the other. Cholic acid or its compounds, the bile acids, are usually detected in suspected liquids by the well-known Pettenkofer reaction. As usually per- formed, the test is made by adding to the liquid a few drops of a 10 per cent, solution of cane-sugar and then strong sulphuric acid. The latter must be added carefully and the temperature be kept below 70° C. If bile acids are present, the liquid assumes a red-violet color. It is now known that the reaction consists in the formation of a substance (furfurol) by the action of the acid on sugar, which then reacts with the bile acids. The bile acids are formed directly in the liver cells. This fact, which was for a long time the subject of discussion, has been demonstrated in recent years by an important series of researches made upon birds. It has been shown that if the bile-duct is ligated in these animals, the bile formed is reabsorbed and bile acids and pigments may be detected in the urine and the blood. If, however, the liver is completely extirpated, then no trace of either bile acids or bile pigments can be found in the blood or the urine, showing that these substances are not formed elsewhere in the body 726 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. than in the liver. It is more difficult to ascertain from what sub- stances they are formed. The fact that glycocoll and taurin contain nitrogen, and that the latter contains sulphur, indicates that some proteid or albuminoid constituent is broken down during their pro- duction. From the standpoint of nutrition the taurocholate is interesting as giving one of the forms in which the sulphur of proteid material is eliminated. Some light has been thrown upon the origin of taurin by the discovery (Friedmann*) that it may be formed from cystin. This latter body, C6H12N2S204, is known to occur as one of the end-products in the acid hydrolysis of proteids, and it is possible that it occurs also in the tryptic-erepsin hydrolysis in the small intestine, representing the end-product in which the sulphur of the proteid molecule is found. Cystin may be oxidized to cysteinic acid (COOHC2H3NH2- S02OH) and from this taurin (C2H4NH2S02OH) may be obtained. It is prob- able, therefore, that the taurin is formed normally from cystin in the body and that the latter represents one of the split products of proteid. f Some of the sulphur of the cystin appears also in the urine in oxidized form as sul- phate. Under certain pathological conditions the cystin itself appears in the urine, giving the phenomenon of cystinuria. A circumstance of considerable physiological significance is that these acids or their decomposition products are absorbed in part from the intestine and are again secreted by the liver; as in the case of the pigments, there is an intestinal-hepatic circulation. The value of this reabsorption may lie in the fact that the bile acids constitute a very efficient stimulus to the bile-secreting activity of the cells, being one of the best of cholagogues, or it may be that it economizes material. From what we know of the history of the bile acids it is evident that they are not to be considered solely as excreta: they have some important function to fulfill. The following suggestions as to their value have been made : In the first place, they serve as a menstruum for dissolving the cholesterin which is constantly present in the bile and which is an excretion to be removed; secondly, they facilitate the splitting and the absorption of fats in the intestine. It is an undoubted fact that when bile is shut off from the intestine the absorption of fats is very much diminished, and it has been shown that this action of the bile in fat absorption is due to the presence of the bile acids. Cholesterin. — Cholesterin is a non-nitrogenous substance of the formula C27H460. It is a constant constituent of the bile, although it occurs in variable quantities. Cholesterin is very widely distrib- uted in the body, being found especially in the white matter (medul- lary substance) of nerve fibers. It seems, moreover, to be a constant constituent of all animal and plant cells. It is assumed that choles- terin is not formed in the liver, but that it is eliminated by the liver cells from the blood, which collects it from the various tissues of the body. According to Naunyn, however, the cholesterin is not secreted * Friedmann, " Hofmeister's Beitrage," 3, 1, 1902. t See Simon, " Johns Hopkins Hospital Bulletin, " 15, 365, 1904. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE LIVER AND SPLEEN. 727 by the liver cells proper, but is added to the secretion while in the bile passages — the gall-ducts and gall-bladder. That it is an excretion is indicated by the fact that it is eliminated unchanged in the feces. Cholesterin is insoluble in water or in dilute saline liquids, and is held in solution in the bile by means of the bile acids. We must regard it as a waste product of cell life, formed probably in minute quantities, and excreted mainly through the liver. It is partly eliminated through the skin, in the sebaceous and sweat secretions, and in the milk. Lecithin, Fats, and Nucleo-albumin. — Lecithin, C^H^NPOg is a compound of glycerophosphoric acid with fatty acid radicals (stearic, oleic, or palmitic) and a nitrogenous base, cholin. When hydrolyzed by boiling with alkali it splits up into these three sub- stances. It is found generally as such, or in combination, in all cells, and evidently plays some as yet unknown part in cell metabolism. It occurs in largest quantity in the white matter of the nervous system. In the liver it occurs to a considerable extent both as lecithin and in a more complex combination with a carbohydrate residue, a compound designated as jecorin. So far as it is found in the bile it represents possibly a waste product derived from the liver or from the body at large. Little is known of its precise physiological sig- nificance. The special importance, if any, of the small proportion of fats and fatty acids in the bile is unknown. The ropy, mucilaginous character of bile is due to the presence of a body formed in the bile- ducts and gall-bladder. This substance was formerly designated as mucin, but it is now known that in ox bile at least it is not a true mucin, but a nucleo-albumin (see appendix). Hammarsten reports that in human bile some true mucin is found. Outside the fact that it makes the bile viscous, this constituent is not known to possess any especial physiological significance. The Secretion of the Bile. — Numerous experiments have been made to ascertain whether or not the secretion of bile is controlled by a special set of secretory fibers. The secretion itself is continuous, but varies in amount under different conditions. These conditions may be controlled experimentally in part. It has been shown, for example, that stimulation of the spinal cord or splanchnic nerve diminishes the flow of bile, while section of the splanchnic branches may cause an increased flow. These and similar actions are ex- plained, however, by their effect on the blood-flow through the liver. The splanchnics carry vasomotor nerves to the liver, and section or stimulation of these nerves will therefore alter the circulation in the organ. Since the secretion increases when the blood-flow is increased and vice versa, it is believed that in this case no special secretory nerve fibers exist. The metabolic processes in the liver cells which 728 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. produce the secretion probably go on at all times, but they are increased when the blood-flow is increased. We may believe, there- fore, that the quantity of the bile secretion varies with the quantity and composition of the blood flowing through the liver. On the physiological and pharmacological side efforts have been made to discover what substances stimulate especially the formation of bile. Such substances are designated as cholagogues. The therapeutical agents capable of giving this action are still a subject of controversy. On the physiological side the following facts are accepted : Any agent that causes an hemolysis of red corpuscles increases the flow of bile, or the same effect is produced if a solution of hemoglobin is injected directly into the blood. This result is in harmony with the views already stated regarding the significance of the bile pigments as an excretory product of hemoglobin. The cholagogue whose action is most distinct and prolonged is bile itself. When fed or injected directly into the circulation bile causes an undoubted increase in the secretion. This effect is due both to the bile acids and bile pigments. Since the bile acids have a hemolytic effect on red corpuscles, it might at first be assumed that their action as cholagogues is due indirectly to this circumstance. The action of the bile acids is, however, much more pronounced than that of other hemolytic agents, and it seems certain, therefore, that they exert a specific effect on the liver cells. Lastly, there is evidence that the secretin, whose action upon the pancreatic secretion has been described, exerts a similar effect upon the secretion of bile. Statements differ somewhat in regard to the extent of this action, but it seems to be certain that, when acids (0.5 per cent. HC1) are injected into the duodenum or upper part of the jejunum, the secretion of bile is increased; and, since this effect takes place when the nervous connections are severed, the effect, as in the case of the pancreatic secretion, is explained by as- suming that the acid converts prosecretin to secretin, and this latter after absorption into the blood acts upon the liver cells.* A similar effect may be obtained by injecting secretin directly into the blood. Since during a meal the stomach normally ejects acid chyme into the duodenum, the importance of this secretin reaction in adapting the secretion of bile to the period of digestion is evident. The Ejection of Bile into the Duodenum — Function of the Gall-bladder. — Although the bile is formed more or less continu- ously, it enters the duodenum periodically during the time of digestion. The secretion during the intervening periods is prevented from enter- ing the duodenum apparently by the fact that the opening of the common bile-duct is closed by a sphincter. The secretion, therefore, backs up into the gall-bladder. According to Bruns,t no bile appears * See Falloise, quoted in Maly's " Jahres-bericht der Thier-chemie, " 33, 611, 1904. f" Archives des sciences biologiques, " 7,87, 1899. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE LIVER AND SPLEEN. 729 in the duodenum as long as the stomach is empty. When, however, a meal is taken the ejection of the chyme into the duodenum is fol- lowed by an ejection of bile.* It would seem, therefore, that each gush of chyme into the duodenum excites, probably by reflex action, a contraction of the gall-bladder. The substances in the chyme that are responsible for the stimulation have been investigated by Bruns. He finds that acids, alkalies, and starches are ineffective, and con- cludes that the reflex is due to the proteids and fats or some of the products of their digestion. The gall-bladder has a muscular coat JO 8 b Z 30' 30' 30' JO' 30' Jo'- JO' 30' 30' 30' 30' 30' 30' 30' 30' t, ^ i 1 , / '\ \ I \ / \ I \ 8 h 2 I I \ ..-' ^ ! V i \ 8 6 Z / / \ i / \ / / i x\x s / \ / Nv / '' ^ \ '-.._ Fig. 264. — Curves showing the velocity of secretion of bile into the duodenum on (1) a diet of milk, uppermost curve; (2) a diet of meat, middle curve; (3) a diet of bread, lowest curve. The divisions on the abscissa represent intervals of thirty minutes; the figures on the ordinates represent the volume of secretion in cubic centimeters. — (Bruns.) of plain muscle, and records made of its contractions show that the force exerted is quite small. According to Freese,f the maximal con- traction does not exceed that necessary to overcome the hydrostatic pressure of a column of water 220 mms. in height, — a force, therefore, which is about equivalent to the secretion pressure of bile as deter- mined by Heidenhain. The innervation of the gall-bladder and gall- ducts has been studied especially by Doyon.} It would seem, from * See also Klodnizki, quoted from Maly's " Jahres-bericht der Thier- chemie," 33, 617, 1904. t "Johns Hopkins Hospital Bulletin," June, 1905. j Doyon, " Archives de physiologie, " 1894, p. 19. 730 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. the experiments made by this author together with later experiments reported by Freese, that the bladder receives both motor and in- hibitory fibers by way of the splanchnic nerves. These fibers emerge from the spinal cord in the roots of the sixth thoracic to the first lumbar spinal nerve, and pass to the celiac plexus by way of the splanchnic nerves. Sensory fibers capable of causing a reflex con- striction or dilatation of the bladder are found in both the vagus and splanchnic nerves. Stimulation of the central end of the cut splanch- nic causes a dilatation of the bladder (reflex stimulation of the inhibitory fibers), while stimulation of the central end of the vagus causes a contraction of the bladder and a dilatation (inhibition) of the sphincter muscle at the opening of the common duct into the in- testine. Since these last movements are the ones that occur during normal digestion, it is probable that the afferent fibers from the duodenum, which are concerned in this reflex, run in the vagus. Effect of Complete Occlusion of the Bile-duct. — It is an in- teresting fact that, when the flow of bile is completely prevented by ligation of the bile-duct, the stagnant liquid is not reabsorbed by the blood directly, but by the lymphatics of the liver. The bile pig- ments and bile acids in such cases may be detected in the lymph as it flows from the thoracic duct. In this way they get into the blood, producing a jaundiced condition. The way in which the bile gets from the bile-ducts into the hepatic lymphatics is not definitely known, but possibly it is due to a rupture, caused by the increased pressure, at some point in the course of the delicate bile capillaries. General Physiological Importance of Bile. — The physiological value of bile has been referred to in speaking of its several constitu- ents. Bile is of importance as an excretion in that it removes from the body waste products of metabolism, such as cholesterin, lecithin, and bile pigments. With reference to the pigments, there is evidence to show that a part at least may be reabsorbed while passing through the intestine, and be used again in some way in the body. The bile acids represent end-products of metabolism involving the proteids of the liver cells, but they are undoubtedly reabsorbed in part, and can not be regarded merely as excreta. As a digestive secretion, the most important function attributed to the bile is the part it takes in the digestion and absorption of fats. It accelerates greatly the action of the lipase of pancreatic juice in splitting the fats to fatty acids and glycerin, and it aids materially in the absorption of the products of this hydrolysis. A number of observers have shown that when a permanent biliary fistula is made, and the bile is thus prevented from reaching the intestinal canal, a large proportion of the fat of the food escapes absorption and is found in the feces. This action of the bile may be referred directly to the fact that the bile acids serve as a solvent for the fats and fatty acids. It was formerly believed that PHYSIOLOGY OF THE LIVER AND SPLEEN. 731 bile is also of great importance in restraining the processes of putre- faction in the intestine. It was asserted that bile is an efficient antiseptic, and that this property comes into use normally in prevent- ing excessive putrefaction. Bacteriological experiments made by a number of observers have shown, however, that bile itself has very feeble antiseptic properties, as is indicated by the fact that it putrefies readily. The free bile acids and cholalic acid do have a direct retard- ing effect upon putrefactions outside the body; but this action is not very pronounced, and has not been demonstrated satisfactorily for bile itself. It seems to be generally true that in cases of biliary fistula the feces have a very fetid odor when meat and fat are taken in the food. But the increased putrefaction in these cases may possibly be an indirect result of the withdrawal of bile. It has been suggested, for instance, that the deficient absorption of fat that follows upon the removal of the bile results in the proteid and carbohydrate material becoming coated with an insoluble layer of fat, so that the penetration of the digestive enzymes is retarded and greater opportunity is given for the action of bacteria. We may conclude, therefore, that, while there does not seem to be sufficient warrant at present for believing that the bile exerts a direct antiseptic action upon the intestinal contents, nevertheless its presence limits in some way the extent of putrefaction. Glycogen. — One of the most important functions of the liver is the formation of glycogen. This substance was found in the liver in 1857 by Claude Bernard, and is one of several brilliant discoveries made by him. Glycogen has the formula (C6H10O5)n, which is also the general formula given to vegetable starch; glycogen is therefore frequently spoken of as ** animal starch." It gives, however, a port- wine-red color with iodin solutions, instead of the familiar deep blue of vegetable starch, and this reaction serves to detect glycogen not only in its solutions, but also in the liver cells. Glycogen is readily soluble in water, and the solutions have a characteristic opalescent appearance.. Like starch, glycogen is acted upon by ptyalin and other diastatic enzymes, and the end-products are apparently the same — namely, maltose, or maltose and some dextrin, or else dex- trose, depending upon the enzyme used. Under the influence of acids it may be hydrolyzed at once to dextrose.* Occurrence of Glycogen in the Liver. — Glycogen can be detected in the liver cells microscopically. If the liver of a dog is removed twelve or fourteen hours after a hearty meal, hardened in alcohol, and sectioned, the liver cells are found to contain clumps of clear material which give the iodin reaction for glycogen. Even *The extensive literature of glycogen is. collected and reviewed by Cre- mer in the u Ergebnisse der Physiologie, " vol. i, part i, 1902; and by Pfluger, " Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie," 96, 1, 1903. 732 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. when distinct aggregations of the glycogen can not be made out, its presence in the cells is shown by the red reaction with iodin. By this simple method one can demonstrate the important fact that the amount of glycogen in the liver increases after meals and decreases again during the fasting hours, and if the fast is sufficiently prolonged it may disappear altogether. This fact is, however, shown more satisfactorily by quantitative determinations, by chemical means, of the total glycogen present. The amount of glycogen in the liver is quite variable, being influenced by such conditions as the character and amount of the food, muscular exercise, body tem- perature, drugs, etc. From determinations made upon various animals it may be said that the average amount lies between 1 .5 and 4 per cent, of the weight of the liver. But this amount may be in- creased greatly by feeding upon a diet largely made up of carbohy- drates. It is said that in the dog the total amount of liver glycogen may be raised to 17 per cent., and in the rabbit to 27 per cent., by this means, while it is estimated for man (Neumeister) that the quan- tity may be increased to at least 10 per cent. It is usually believed that glycogen exists as such in the liver cells, being deposited in the substance of the cytoplasm. Reasons have been brought forward to show that this is not strictly true, and that the glycogen is prob- ably held in some sort of weak chemical combination. It has been shown, for instance, that although glycogen is easily soluble in cold water, it can not be extracted readily from the liver cells by this agent. One must use hot water, salts of the heavy metals, and other similar agents that may be supposed to break up the combination in which the glycogen exists. For practical purposes, however, we may speak of the glycogen as lying free in the liver-cells, just as we speak of hemoglobin existing as such in the red corpuscles, although it is probably held in some sort of combination. Origin of Glycogen. — To understand clearly the views held as to the origin of liver glycogen, it is necessary to describe briefly the effect of the different foodstuffs upon its formation. Effect of Carbohydrates on the Amount of Glycogen. — The amount of glycogen in the liver is affected very quickly by the quantity of car- bohydrates in the food. If the carbohydrates are given in excess, the supply of glycogen may be increased largely beyond the average amount present, as has been stated above. Investigation of the differ- ent sugars has shown that dextrose, levulose, saccharose (cane-sugar), and maltose are unquestionably direct glycogen-formers, — that is, that glycogen is formed directly from them or from the products into which they are converted during digestion. The bulk of our car- bohydrate food reaches the liver as dextrose, or as dextrose and levu- lose, and these forms of sugar may be converted into glycogen in the liver cells by a simple process of dehydration, such as may be repre- PHYSIOLOGY OF THE LIVER AND SPLEEN. 733 sented in substance by the formula C6H1206 — H20 = C6H10O5. There is no doubt that both dextrose and levulose increase markedly the amount of glycogen in the liver; and, since cane-sugar is inverted in the intestine before absorption, it also must be a true glycogen- former, — a fact that has been abundantly demonstrated by direct experiment. Lusk* has shown, however, that, if cane-sugar is in- jected under the skin, it has a very feeble effect in the way of increas- ing the amount of glycogen in the liver, since under these conditions it is probably absorbed into the blood without undergoing inversion. Experiments with subcutaneous injection of lactose gave similar results, and it is generally believed that the liver cells can not convert the double sugars to glycogen, at least not readily; hence the value of the hydrolysis of these sugars in the alimentary canal before absorption. We may assume, therefore, that dextrose, levulose, and galactose are the true glycogen-formers that occur normally in the blood, and that the disaccharids (cane-sugar, milk-sugar, etc.) and the polysaccharids (starches) are true glycogen-formers to the ex- tent that they are converted into dextrose, levulose, or galactose. Effect of Proteid on Glycogen Formation. — In his first studies upon glycogen Bernard asserted that it may be formed from proteid material. Since that time there have been much discussion and experimentation upon this point. The usual view is that proteid must be counted among the true glycogen-formers in the sense that some of the material of the proteid molecule is directly converted to glycogen. The proteid in digestion undergoes, it will be remembered, a splitting process the limits of which are not definitely settled. It is assumed, however, that the nitrogenous split products are acted upon in the liver, the nitrogen with some of the carbon being changed to urea, while the non-nitrogenous residue is converted to sugar. Among the split products of proteid that have been especially investigated in this relation the results with leucin and glucosamin have been chiefly negative.! Experimentally observers find for the warm-blooded animals at least that feeding with proteids, even in the case of those proteids, such as casein, that con- tain no carbohydrate grouping, causes an increased production of glycogen. % The conclusion to be drawn from these experiments is strengthened by clinical experience upon human beings suf- fering from diabetes. In severe forms of this disease all the car- bohydrate material of the food appears in the urine. If under these conditions the individual is given an exclusively proteid diet sugar still continues to appear in the urine, and it would seem that this sugar can only arise from the proteid food. In the similar * Voit, " Zeitschrift f. Biologie, " 28, 285, 1891. t Halsey, " American Journal of Physiology, " 10, 229, 1904. % See Stookey, " American Journal of Physiology, " 9, 138, 1903. 734 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. condition of severe glycosuria that may be produced by the use of phloridzin it has been shown that the animal continues to excrete sugar even when fed on proteid alone, or when starved. Under such conditions the amount of dextrose in the urine bears a definite ratio to the amount of nitrogen excreted. D : N :: 3.65 : 1 (Lusk), which would indicate that both arise from the breaking down of the proteid molecule. On this supposition 58.4 per cent, of the proteid may be converted to sugar. So also the fact that during prolonged starva- tion, lasting for forty or even ninety days, the blood retains a practi- cally constant composition in sugar indicates that this material is being formed from either the proteid or fat supply of the body. Other considerations exclude the fat, and we are, therefore, led to the belief that the proteid can give rise to sugar in the body. If this change is part of the normal metabolism of the body it would make proteid a glycogen-former, since the sugar formed from the proteid may, of course, be converted to glycogen. Whether or not all proteids yield glycogen or sugar in the body is not entirely de- termined. Some authors have thought that only those proteids that contain a carbohydrate residue have this property; but, as stated above, casein and other proteids that do not possess this grouping seem also to increase the glycogen supply when fed alone. Effect of Fats upon Glycogen Formation. — A large number of substances have been found by some observers to increase the store of glycogen in the liver. In some of these cases at least it is evident that the substance is not a direct glycogen-former in the sense that the material is itself converted to glycogen. It may increase the supply of liver glycogen in some indirect way, — for example, by diminishing the consumption of glycogen in the body. The most important substance in this connection from a practical standpoint is fat. Whether or not the body can convert fats into sugar or glycogen is a question about- which at present there is much difference of opinion, and much evidence might be cited on each side. Cremer, however, has furnished apparent proof that glycerin acts as a direct glycogen or sugar-former. When fed, especially in the diabetic condition, it causes an increase in the sugar which can not be explained as a result of proteid metabolism. Since in the body neutral fats are normally split into glycerin and fatty acid, the fact that glycerin can be converted to sugar seems to carry with it the admission that fats may contribute directly to sugar production. Whether the synthesis of sugar (or glycogen) from glycerin is, so to speak, a normal process or occurs only under especial condi- tions, can not be decided at present. The Function of Glycogen — Glycogenic Theory. — The mean- ing of the formation of glycogen in the liver has been, and still is, the subject of discussion. The view advanced first by Bernard is PHYSIOLOGY OF THE LIVER AND SPLEEN. 735 perhaps most generally accepted. According to Bernard, glycogen forms a temporary reserve supply of carbohydrate material that is laid up in the liver during digestion and is gradually made use of in the intervals between meals. During digestion the carbohydrate food is absorbed into the blood of the portal system as dextrose or as dextrose, levulose, and galactose. If these sugars passed through the liver unchanged, the contents of the systemic blood in sugar would be increased perceptibly. It is now known that when the percentage of sugar in the blood rises above a certain normal limit (condition of hyperglycemia), the excess is excreted through the kidney and is lost. But as the blood from the digestive organs passes through the liver the excess of sugar is abstracted by the liver cells, is dehydrated to make glycogen, and is retained in the cells in this form for a short period. From time to time the glycogen is reconverted into sugar (dextrose) and is given off to the blood. By this means the percentage of sugar in the systemic blood is kept nearly constant (0.1 to 0.2 per cent.) and within limits best adapted to the use of the tissues. The great importance of the formation of glycogen and the consequent conservation of the sugar supply of the tissues is evident when we consider the nutritive value of carbohydrate food. Carbohydrates form the bulk of our usual diet, and the proper regulation of the supply to the tissues is, there- fore, of vital importance in the maintenance of a normal, healthy condition. The second part of this theory, which holds that the glycogen is reconverted to dextrose, is supported by observations upon livers removed from the body. It has been found that shortly after the removal of the liver the supply of glycogen begins to dis- appear and a corresponding increase in dextrose occurs. Within a comparatively short time all the glycogen is gone and only dextrose is found. It is for this reason that in the estimation of glycogen in the liver it is necessary to mince the organ and to throw it into boiling water as quickly as possible, since by this means the liver cells are killed and the conversion of the glycogen is stopped. How the gly- cogen is changed to dextrose by the liver is a matter not fully ex- plained. According to most authors, the conversion is due to an enzyme produced in the liver. Extracts of liver, as of some other tissues, yield a diastatic enzyme that changes glycogen to dextrose.* It is probable, therefore, that the normal conversion of glycogen to dextrose is effected by a special enzyme produced in the liver cells. In this description of the origin and meaning of the liver glycogen reference has been made only to the glycogen derived directly from digested carbohydrates. The glycogen derived from proteid foods, once it is formed in the liver, has, of course, the same functions to fulfill. It is converted into sugar, and eventually is oxidized in * Tebb, " Journal of Physiology, " 22, 423, 1897-98. 736 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. ' the tissues. For the sake of completeness it may be well to add that some of the sugar of the blood formed from the glycogen may under certain conditions be converted into fat in the adipose tissues, instead of being burnt, and in this way it may be retained in the body as a reserve supply of food of a more stable character. Glycogen in the Muscles and other Tissues. — The history of glycogen is not complete without some reference to its occurrence in the muscles. Glycogen is, in fact, found in various places in the body, and is widely distributed throughout the animal kingdom. It occurs, for example, in leucocytes, in the placenta, in the rapidly growing tissues of the embryo, and in considerable abundance in the oyster and other molluscs. But in our bodies and in those of the mammals generally the most significant occurrence of glycogen, outside the liver, is in the voluntary muscles, of which glycogen forms a normal constituent. It has been estimated that the percentage of glycogen in resting muscle varies from 0.5 to 0.9 per cent., and that in the musculature of the whole body there may be contained an amount of glycogen equal to that in the liver itself. Apparently muscular tissue, as well as liver tissue, has a glycogenetic function — that is, it is capable of laying up a supply of glycogen from the sugar brought to it by the blood. The glycogenetic function of muscle has been demonstrated directly by Kulz,* who has shown that an isolated muscle irrigated with an artificial supply of blood to which dextrose is added is capable of changing the dextrose to glycogen, as shown by the increase in the latter substance in the muscle after irriga- tion. Muscle glycogen is to be looked upon as a temporary and local reserve supply of material; so that, while we have in the liver a large general depot for the temporary storage of glycogen for the use of the body at large, the muscular tissue, which, considering its bulk, is the most active tissue of the body from the standpoint of energy production, is also capable of laying up in the form of glycogen any excess of sugar brought to it. The fact that glycogen occurs so widely in the rapidly growing cells of embryos indicates that this glycogenetic function may at times be exercised by any tissue. Conditions Affecting the Supply of Glycogen in Muscle and Liver. — In accordance with the view given above of the general value of glycogen — namely, that it is a temporary reserve supply of carbohydrate material that may be rapidly converted to sugar and oxidized with the liberation of energy — it is found that the supply of glycogen is greatly affected by conditions calling for increased metabolism in the body. Muscular exercise quickly exhausts the supply of muscle and liver glycogen, provided it is not renewed by new food. In a starving animal glycogen finally disappears, * "Zeitschrift f. Biologie, " 27, 237, 1890. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE LIVER AND SPLEEN. 737 except perhaps in traces, but this disappearance occurs much sooner if the animal is made to use its muscles at the same time. It has been shown also by Morat and Dufourt that, if a muscle has been made to contract vigorously, it takes up much more sugar from an artificial supply of blood sent through it than a similar muscle which has been resting; on the other hand, it has been found that if the nerve of one leg is cut so as to paralyze the muscles of that side of the body, the amount of glycogen is greater in these muscles than in those of the other leg that have been contracting meantime and using up their glycogen. The further history of glycogen is considered in the section on nutrition. Formation of Urea in the Liver. — The nitrogen contained in the proteid material of our food is finally eliminated, after the metab- olism of the proteid is completed, mainly in the form of urea. It has been definitively proved that the urea is not formed in the kidneys, the organs that eliminate it. It has long been considered a matter of the greatest importance to ascertain in what organ or tissues urea is formed. Investigations have gone so far as to demonstrate that it arises in part at least in the liver; hence the property of forming urea must be added to the other important functions of the liver cell. Schroder* performed a number of experiments in which the liver was taken from a freshly killed dog and irrigated through its blood- vessels with a supply of blood obtained from another dog. If the supply of blood was taken from a fasting animal, then circulating it through the isolated liver was not followed by any increase in the amount of urea contained in it. If, on the contrary, the blood was obtained from a well-fed dog, the amount of urea contained in it was distinctly increased by passing it through the liver, thus indicating that the blood of an animal after digestion contains something that the liver can convert to urea. It is to be noted, moreover, that this power is not possessed by all the organs, since blood from well-fed animals showed no increase in urea after being circulated through an isolated kidney or muscle. As further proof of the urea-forming power of the liver Schroder found that if ammonium carbonate was added to the blood circulating through the liver — to that from the fasting as well as from the well-nourished animal — a very decided increase in the urea was always obtained. It follows from the last experiment that the liver cells are able to convert carbonate of ammonium into urea. The reaction may be expressed by the equa- tion (NH4)2C03 — 2H20 = CON2H4. Schondorff f in some later work showed that if the blood of a fasting dog is irrigated through the hind legs of a well-nourished animal, no increase in urea in the blood can * "Archiv f. experimentelle Pathologie und Pharmakologie, " 15, 364, 1882, and 19, 373, 1885. t " Pfliiger's Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 54, 420, 1893. 47 738 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. be detected; but if the blood, after irrigation through the hind legs, is subsequently passed through the liver, a marked increase in urea results. Obviously, the blood in this experiment derives something from the tissues of the leg which the tissues themselves can not convert to urea, but which the liver cells can. Finally, in some remarkable experiments upon dogs made by four investigators (Hahn, Massen, Nencki, and Pawlow), which are described more fully in the next chapter, it was shown that when the liver is practically destroyed there is a distinct diminution in the urea of the urine. In birds uric acid takes the place of urea as the main nitrogenous ex- cretion of the body, and Minkowski has shown that in them removal of the liver is followed by an important diminution in the amount of uric acid excreted. From experiments such as these it is safe to conclude that urea is formed in the liver and is then given to the blood and excreted by the kidney. In treating of the physiological history of urea an account will be given of the views proposed with regard to the antecedent substance or substances from which the liver produces urea. Physiology of the Spleen. — Much has been said and written about the spleen, but we are yet in the dark as to the distinctive function or functions of this organ. The few facts that are known may be stated briefly without going into the details of theories that have been offered at one time or another. The older experimenters demonstrated that this organ may be removed from the body without serious injury to the animal. An increase in the size of the lymph- glands and of the bone-marrow has been stated to occur after ex- tirpation; but this is denied by others, and, whether true or not, it gives but little clue to the normal functions of the spleen. Some observers* find that the removal of the spleen causes a marked diminution in the number of red corpuscles and the quantity of hemoglobin. They infer, therefore, that the spleen is normally concerned in some way in the formation of red corpuscles. Others, however, report with equal positiveness that removal of the spleen has no effect upon the number of red corpuscles or upon the power of the animal to regenerate its corpuscles after hemorrhage^ The most definite facts known about the spleen are in connection with its movements. It has been shown that there is a slow expansion and contraction 'of the organ synchronous with the digestion periods. After a meal the spleen begins to increase in size, reaching a maximum at about the fifth hour, and then slowly returns to its previous size. This movement, the meaning of which is not known, is probably due to a slow vasodilatation, together, perhaps, with a relaxation of the tonic contraction of the musculature of the trabecular. In addition *Laudenbach, " Centralblatt fur Physiologie, " 9, 1, 1895. t Paton, Gulland, and Fowler, " Journal of Physiology, " 28, 83, 1902. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE LIVER AND SPLEEN. 739 to this slow movement, Roy* has shown that there is a rhythmical contraction and relaxation of the organ, occurring in cats and dogs at intervals of about one minute. Roy supposes that these con- tractions are effected through the intrinsic musculature of the organ, — that is, the plain muscle tissue present in the capsule and trabecular, — and he believes that the contractions serve to keep up a circulation through the spleen and to make its vascular supply more or less independent of variations in general arterial pressure. The fact that there is a special local arrangement for maintaining its cir- culation makes the spleen unique among the organs of the body, but no light is thrown upon the nature of the function fulfilled. The spleen is supplied richly with motor nerve fibers which when stimu- lated either directly or reflexly cause the organ to diminish in volume. According to Schaefer,* these fibers are contained in the splanchnic nerves, which carry also inhibitory fibers whose stimu- lation produces a dilatation of the spleen. The chemical composition of the spleen is complicated, but sug- gestive. Its mineral constituents are characterized by a large percentage of iron, which seems to be present as an organic compound of some kind. Analysis shows also the presence of a number of fatty acids, fats, cholesterin, and, what is perhaps more noteworthy, a number of nitrogenous extractives belonging to the group of purin bases, such as xanthin, hypoxanthin, adenin, guanin, and uric acid. The presence of these bodies seems to indicate that active metabolic changes of some kind occur in the spleen. As to the theories of the splenic functions, the following may be mentioned: (1) The spleen has been supposed to give rise to new red corpuscles. This it un- doubtedly does during fetal life and shortly after birth, and in some animals throughout life, but there is no reliable evidence that the function is retained in adult life in man or in most of the mammals. (2) It has been supposed' to be an organ for the destruction of red corpuscles. This view is founded chiefly on microscopical evidence according to which certain large ameboid cells in the spleen ingest and destroy the old red corpuscles, and partly upon the fact that the spleen tissue seems to be rich in an iron-containing compound. This theory can not be considered at present as satisfactorily demon- strated. (3) It has been suggested that the spleen is concerned in the production of uric acid. This substance is found in the spleen, as stated above, and it has been shown by Horbaczewsky that the spleen contains a substance from which uric acid or xanthin may readily be formed ; but further investigation has shown that the same substance is found in lymphoid tissue generally. If, therefore, uric acid is produced in the spleen, it probably originates in the large * "Journal of Physiology," 3, 203, 1881. t "Journal of Physiology," 20, 1, 1896. 740 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. amount of lymphoid tissue contained in it, and is not a product char- acteristic of the splenic tissue proper. In this connection it may be stated that Jones* has demonstrated the existence in the spleen of an enzyme, adenase, which converts adenin, C5H3N4NH2, into hypoxan- thin, C5H3N4OH. The lymphoid tissue of the spleen must also possess the property of producing lymphocytes, since, according to the general view, these corpuscles are formed in lymphoid tissue generally wherever the so-called "germ-centers" occur. (4) Lastly, a theory has been supported by Schiff and Herzen, according to which the spleen produces something (an enzyme) which, when carried in the blood to the pancreas, acts upon the trypsinogen con- tained in this gland, converting it into trypsin. This view has been corroborated by a number of observers, but it is difficult at present to decide whether such an action occurs normally during digestion. As already stated, the general testimony at present indicates that the pancreatic juice when secreted contains its trypsin in inactive form. It is activated only after reaching the duodenum under the influence of the enterokinase. * Jones and Winternitz, " Zeitschrift, f. physiol. Chemie," 44, 1, 1905. CHAPTER XLV. THE KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. Structure of the Kidney. — The kidney is a compound tubular gland. The uriniferous tubules composing it may be roughly separated into a secreting part comprising the capsule, convoluted tubes, and loop of Henle, and a collecting part, the so-called straight or collecting tube, the epithelium of which is assumed not to have any secretory function. Within the secreting part the epithe- lium differs greatly in character in different regions; its peculiarities may be referred to briefly here so far as they seem to have a physio- Fig. 265. — Portions of the various divisions of the uriniferous tubules drawn from sections of human kidney: A, Malpighian body; x, squamous epithelium lining the cap- sule and reflected over the glomerulus; y, z, afferent and efferent vessels of the tuft; e, nuclei of capillaries; n, constricted neck marking passage of capsule into convoluted tu- bule; B, proximal convoluted tubule; C, irregular tubule; D and F, spiral tubules; E, ascending limb of Henle's loop; G, straight collecting tubule. — (Piersol.) logical bearing, although for a complete description reference must be made to works on histology. The arrangement of the glandular epithelium in the capsule with reference to the blood-vessels of the glomerulus is worthy of special attention. It will be remembered that each Malpighian corpuscle con- sists of two principal parts, a tuft of blood-vessels, the glomerulus, and an enveloping expansion of the uriniferous tubule, the capsule. The glomerulus is an interesting structure (see Fig. 265, A). It consists 741 742 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. of a small afferent artery which after entering the glomerulus, breaks up into a number of capillaries. These capillaries, although twisted together, do not anastomose, and they unite to form a single efferent vein of a smaller diameter than the afferent artery. The whole structure, therefore, is not an ordinary capillary area, but a rete mirdbile, and the physical factors are such that within the capil- laries of the rete there must be a greatly diminished velocity of the blood-stream, owing to the great increase in the width of the stream bed, and a high blood-pressure as compared with ordinary capil- laries. Surrounding this glomerulus is the double-walled capsule. One wall of the capsule is closely adherent to the capillaries of the glomerulus; it not only covers the structure closely, but dips into the interior between the small lobules into which the glomerulus is divided. This layer of the capsule is composed of flattened, endo- thelial-like cells, the glomerular epithelium, to which great impor- tance is attached in the formation of the secretion. It will be no- ticed that between the interior of the blood-vessels of the glomerulus and the cavity of the capsule, which is the beginning of the urin- iferous tubule, there are interposed only two very thin layers, — namely, the epithelium of the capillary wall and the glomerular epithelium. The apparatus would seem to afford most favorable conditions for nitration of the liquid parts of the blood. The epi- thelium clothing the convoluted portions of the tubule, including under this designation the so-called irregular and spiral portions and the loop of Henle, is of a character quite different from that of the glomerular epithelium (Fig. 265, B, C, D, E, F, G). The cells, speaking generally, are cuboidal or cylindrical, protoplasmic, and granular in appearance; on the side toward the basement mem- brane they often show a peculiar striation, while on the lumen side the extreme periphery presents a compact border which in some cases shows a cilia-like striation. These cells have the general appearance of an active secretory epithelium, and one theory of urinary secretion attributes this function to them. The Secretion of Urine. — The kidneys receive a rich supply of nerve fibers, but most histologists have been unable to trace any connection between these fibers and the epithelial cells of the kidney tubules. The majority of purely physiological experiments upon direct stimulation of the nerves going to the kidney are adverse to the theory of secretory fibers, the marked effects obtained in these ex- periments being all explicable by the changes produced in the blood- flow through the organ. Two general theories of urinary secretion have been proposed. Ludwig held that the urine is formed by the simple physical processes of filtration and diffusion. In the glom- eruli the conditions are most favorable to filtration, and he sup- KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 743 posed that in these structures water filtered through from the blood, carrying with it not only the inorganic salts, but also the specific elements (urea, etc.) of the secretion. There was thus formed at the beginning of the uriniferous tubules a complete but diluted urine, and in the subsequent passage of this liquid along the con- voluted tubes it became concentrated by diffusion with the more concentrated lymph surrounding the outside of the tubules. Bowman's theory of urinary secretion, which has since been vigorously supported and extended by Heidenhain, was based orig- inally mainly on histological grounds. It assumes that in the glomeruli water and inorganic salts are produced, while the urea and related bodies are eliminated through the activity of the epi- thelial cells in the convoluted tubes. The first of these theories (Ludwig) is sometimes spoken of as the mechanical theory, since as originally proposed it attempted to explain the formation and composition of the urine by reference only to the physical forces of filtration and diffusion. Adherents of this view in recent years have modified it, however, to the extent that the absorption supposed to take place in the convoluted tubules is designated as a selective absorption, or selective diffusion, the characteristics of which depend upon unknown peculiarities of struc- ture in the epithelial cell, so that it is no longer a purely mechanical theory. The difference between the Ludwig and the Bowman theories may be stated briefly in this way. The former assumes that in the glomerulus all of the constituents of the urine are produced from the blood, probably by filtration, and that the function of the epithelium lining the convoluted tubules is absorptive, like the epithelium of the intestines, and not secretory. The Bowman view as formulated by Heidenhain teaches that the glomerular epithe- lium forms the water and salts of the urine by an act of secretion, the ultimate chemistry or physics of which is not known, but which implies that the epithelial cells participate in the process and do not act simply as a passive membrane. The cells of the convoluted tubules are also secretory, their special activity being limited mainly to the organic constituents, urea, etc., although, in this respect, — namely, in the precise distinction between the secretory products of the glomerular epithelium and those of the convoluted tubules, — the theory is not very explicit. Much interest and a large literature have been stimulated by the controversies between these theories, and to-day the facts accumulated are not such as to demonstrate conclusively one view or the other, although, on the whole, perhaps, it may be said that the majority of physiologists adhere to the more conservative view of Bowman-Heidenhain to the extent at least of recognizing that the physical laws of filtration, diffusion, and inhibition, so far as they are known, do not suffice for a satis- 744 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. factory explanation of the facts. As in similar cases, our knowl- edge of the physical structure and chemical properties of the wall of living cells is still very deficient, and it seems necessary to desig- nate the activities of this wall by the indefinite term secretion. Function of the Glomerulus. — As stated above, the structure of the glomerulus is peculiar and suggestive of a special adaptation. Ludwig's theory looks upon it as a filter, the pressure of the blood in the glomerular capillaries driving the water and salts through the endothelium of the capillaries and the glomerular epithe- lium into the cavity of the urinary tubule. If we consider only the water and assume that the membranes traversed are freely permeable to its molecules, then it is evident that, upon this theory, the quantity of urine formed will depend upon the filtration pres- sure, and that this filtration pressure can be expressed by the formula F=P — p, in which P represents the blood-pressure in the glom- erular capillaries and p the pressure of the urine in the capsular end of the uriniferous tubules. Some of the interesting facts de- veloped by experiment may be presented in connection with this formula. According to the mechanical theory, the amount of urine formed should vary directly with P and inversely with p. The factor P may be increased in two general ways: First, by those changes which raise general arterial pressure and therefore the pressure in the renal arteries, — such changes, for instance, as are brought about by an increased force of heart beat or a large vaso- constriction. Second, by obstructing or occluding the renal veins. Experiments have been made along these lines. With regard to the first possibility it has been found in general that raising arterial pressure increases the quantity of urine if the means used are such as may be assumed to raise the pressure in the glomerular capillaries. The reverse experiment, however, of raising P by blocking the venous outflow fails entirely to support the theory. When the renal veins are compressed the capillary pressure in the glomeruli must be increased, and, if the veins are blocked entirely, we may suppose that the capillary pressure is raised to the level of that of the renal arteries. In such experiments, however, the flow of urine is di- minished instead of being increased, and indeed may be stopped altogether when the veins are completely blocked. The adherents of the Ludwig theory have attempted to explain this unfavorable result by assuming that the swollen interlobular veins press upon and block the uriniferous tubules. According to the antagonistic theory of Heidenhain, blocking the veins suppresses the secretory activity of the glomerular epithelium by depriving it of oxygen and the chance for removal of C02, — that is, by producing local as- phyxia. The latter explanation seems the simpler of the two, and it is very strongly supported by the opposite experiment of clamping KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 745 the renal artery. When this is done the blood-flow through the kidney ceases and the secretion of urine also stops, as would be expected. But when after a few minutes' closure the artery is un- damped, the secretion is not restored with the return of the cir- culation. On the contrary, a long time (as much as an hour or more) may elapse before the secretion begins. This fact is quite in harmony with the Heidenhain theory, since complete removal of their blood supply might well result in a long-continued injury to the delicate epithelial cells. On the mechanical theory, however, we should expect the contrary. Injury to the cells should be followed by greater permeability and an increased filtration, as is found to be the case with the production of lymph. These two experiments, blocking the renal artery and the renal vein, seem at present to dis- credit the filtration theory and to support the secretion theory. If we accept this latter theory it may be asked how it agrees with the experiments mentioned above upon the variations in capillary pressure brought about otherwise than by obstructing the venous outflow. Heidenhain has emphasized the fact that all of these ex- periments involve not only a variation in capillary pressure, but also in the blood-flow, and that it is open to us to suppose that the effect upon the secretion of urine is dependent upon the rate of flow rather than upon the capillary pressure. If we adopt this expla- nation we are led again to the secretion hypothesis. Mere rate of flow should not influence filtration, but might affect secretion, since it would alter the composition of the blood flowing through the glomeruli and also the amount of oxygen and carbon dioxid. An important fact, which seems at first sight to show a direct influence of pressure, is that when general arterial pressure falls below a cer- tain point, about 40 nuns, of mercury, the secretion of urine ceases altogether. Such a condition may be brought about by surgical shock, by hemorrhage, or by section of the spinal cord in the cer- vical or thoracic region. But here again the great vascular dila- tation causing this fall of pressure is associated with a feeble cir- culation, and the effect upon the kidney secretion may well be due to this latter factor. In addition to varying the factor P in the formula given above, it is possible also to increase the factor p. Normally the pressure of the urine in the capsule must be very low, owing to the fact that the secretion drains away as rapidly as it is formed. If the ureter is occluded, however, the pressure of the urine will increase, and the filtration pressure P — p will diminish. When this experiment is performed and the pressure in the ureter is measured by a manom- eter, it is found to rise to 50 or 60 mms. of mercury and then to remain stationary. This fact might be explained by supposing that when p=P the secretion stops on account of the failure of 746 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. the filtration pressure. Little weight, however, can be given to this argument, since it is quite possible that under these condi- tions the urine may still continue to form, but be reabsorbed under the high tension reached. The experiment simply serves to show the secretion pressure of the urine, and the fact that this pressure rises as high as 50 to 60 mms. mercury, while the capillary pressure is probably somewhat lower, would rather serve as an argument against the filtration theory. Exact figures, however, regard- ing the capillary pressure in the kidney can not be obtained; so that the experiment, on the whole, gives us no satisfactory in- formation regarding the theory of secretion. Dreser has used a different argument to prove that the production of the water in- volves the performance of work on the part of the epithelial cells. He points out that in some conditions — e. g., after drinking beer — the urine may be very dilute, as shown by the fact that its freezing point may be only 0.18° C. or 0.16° C. below that of pure water, — that is, A = —0.18° C. or —0.16° C. (see appendix). Since blood- serum has A = — 0.56° C, the difference in concentration between the blood and the urine in such a case of extreme dilution shows an osmotic pressure in favor of the blood equivalent to A = — 0.4° C. Measured in mechanical units, this would indicate an initial osmotic pressure of 49.08 meters of water tending to drive the water from the uriniferous tubules into the blood, whereas the filtration pres- sure driving the water in the other direction could not at a max- imum exceed 2.72 meters of water. If this argument is valid, the elimination of the water takes place against a strong opposing os- motic pressure, and the energy necessary for its secretion can be referred only to the activity of the epithelial cells. Function of the Convoluted Tubule. — By the term convoluted tubule is meant here the entire stretch from the glomerulus to the straight tubules. Its epithelium varies in character; its cells are distinguished in general, as contrasted with the glomerular epithe- lium, by a relatively large amount of granular protoplasm. The question of interest at present in regard to this epithelium is whether it is secretory or absorptive. The original view of Ludwig that diffusion takes place in these tubules between the urine and the blood (lymph) in accordance with simple physical laws and that by this action alone the dilute urine is brought to its normal concen- tration must be abandoned. The mere fact that the urine may be more concentrated in certain constituents than the blood is suffi- cient evidence that other factors must co-operate. Those who be- lieve that the main function of the tubules is absorptive are obliged to regard this process as physiological, as a selective absorption depending upon the living structure and properties of the epithelial cells. The kind of evidence upon which this view is based is some- KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 747 what indirect; a single example may suffice. Cushny states* that if certain diuretics — for example, sodium chlorid and sodium sul- phate— are injected simultaneously into the blood and in such amounts that an equal number of the anions (CI and S04) are pres- ent, the quantities that are excreted in the urine during the next hour or two follow different curves and vary independently of their concentration in the plasma. While this independence might be referred to a specific secretory action, the author finds a simpler explanation in variations in absorption, the epithelium of the con- voluted tubule, like that of the intestine, absorbing the sulphate with more difficulty. On the other side, the facts that have been urged in favor of the secretory hypothesis are more numerous and varied, but none is entirely convincing. Some of these facts are as follows: (1) It is stated that if the ureters are ligated in birds the urates will be found deposited in the uriniferous tubules, but never at the capsular end. (2) Heidenhain has given proof that the convoluted tubules are capable of excreting indigo-carmin after this substance is injected into the blood. His experiment consisted essentially in injecting the material into the blood, after dividing the cord so as to reduce the rapidity of secretion. After a certain interval the kidney was removed and irrigated with alcohol to pre- cipitate the indigo-carmin in situ in the organ. Microscopical ex- amination showed that after this treatment the granules of the indigo-carmin are found in the convoluted tubules, but not in the capsules around the glomeruli. (3) Several observers (Van der Stricht, Disse, Trambasti, Gurwitschf) have described microscopical appearances in the cells lining the tubules indicative of an active secretion. They picture the formation of vesicles in the cells and appearances which indicate the discharge of these vesicles into the cavity of the tubules. (4) Nussbaum made use of the fact that in the frog the glomeruli are supplied by branches of the renal artery, while the rest of the tubes are supplied by the renal portal vein. He stated that if the renal artery is ligated the glomeruli are de- prived completely of blood, and that as a result the flow of urine ceases. If under these conditions urea is injected into the circulation it is excreted together with some water, thus proving the secretory activity of the tubules with regard to urea. Later experiments by Adami and by Beddard % have thrown doubt upon this otherwise crucial experiment. Adami claims that ligature of the renal arte- ries does not shut off completely the glomerular circulation, while Beddard, although he corroborates Nussbaum in the point that complete occlusion of the renal arteries suspends entirely the secre- * "Journal of Physiology," 27, 429, 1902. t See Gurwitsch, " Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 91, 71, 1902.- t Beddard, " Journal of Physiology, " 28, 20, 1902. 748 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. tion of urine, finds that under these conditions injection of urea into the circulation is not followed by a secretion. (5) Dreser has shown that the acidity of the urine is due to an action of the epithelium of the tubules. If an acid indicator, such as acid fuchsin, is in- jected into the dorsal lymph sac of a frog, and an hour or so later the kidneys are examined, it will be found that the convoluted tu- bules are colored red, while the capsular end is colorless, indicating that the secretion at the latter point has an alkaline reaction. The experiment shows that the acid substances in the urine are produced in the convoluted tubules. The simplest explanation is that they are formed by a secretory activity of the epithelial cells, although one may adopt the less probable view that the cells produce the acid phosphates by a selective absorption of alkaline salts. On the whole, it must be admitted that the weight of evidence is in favor of the Bowman-Heidenhain theory of secretion, and it remains for future investigations to explain more definitely what is meant by the obscure term secretory activity. Under pathological conditions it has been shown satisfactorily that the albumin and sugar which may be present in the urine are secreted or eliminated at the glomerular end of the tubule. Action of Diuretics. — An important side of the theories of secretion of urine is their application to the action of diuretics. Water; various soluble substances, such as salts, urea, and dextrose; and certain special drugs, such as caffein or digitalis, exert a diuretic action on the kidneys. Much experimental work has been done to ascertain whether the action of these substances can be explained mechanically by their influence on the blood-flow or the blood- pressure in the kidney capillaries, or whether it is necessary to fall back upon a specific stimulating effect exerted by them upon the epithelial cells of the tubules. Adherents of the original Ludwig theory are forced to explain their action by the effect they pro- duce upon the pressure in the kidney capillaries, and, indeed, it has been shown with reference to the saline diuretics that their effect upon the secretion is in proportion to the osmotic pressure they exert. It has been suggested, therefore, that the action of these diuretics lies in the fact that they attract water from the tis- tues into the blood and thus cause a condition of hydremic plethora. But whether the elimination of this excess of water is due to filtra- tion or to an active secretion by the glomerular epithelium is a question that revives the discussion that has been presented briefly above. Most observers find that the vascular changes in the kid- ney, particularly after the administration of caffein and digitalis, do not explain satisfactorily the phenomenon of diuresis, and al- though it is necessary to admit that the diuretics, or some of them, act in part by the changes which they cause in the circulation in KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 749 the kidney, those who adopt the Bowman-Heidenhain theory as- sume usually that these substances exert also a direct stimulating action on the secretory cells. The Blood-flow through the Kidneys. — It will be inferred from the discussion above that, other conditions remaining the same, the secretion of the kidney varies with the quantity of blood flowing through it. It is, therefore, important to refer briefly to the nature and especially the regulation of the blood-flow through this organ, although the same subject is referred to in connection with the general description of vasomotor regulation (see Circulation). It has been shown by Landergren* and Tigerstedt that the kidney is a very vascular organ, at least when it is in strong functional activ- ity such as may be produced by the action of diuretics. They esti- mate that in a minute's time, under the action of diuretics, an amount of blood flows through the kidney equal to the weight of the organ; this is an amount from four to nineteen times as great as occurs in the average supply of the other organs in the systemic circulation. Taking both kidneys into account, their figures show that (in strong diuresis) 5.6 per cent, of the total quantity of blood sent out of the left heart in a minute may pass through the kidneys, although the combined weight of these organs makes only 0.56 per cent, of that of the body. The nature of the supply of vasomotor nerves to the kidney and the conditions which bring them into activity are fairly well known, owing to the useful invention of the oncometer by Roy. This in- strument is, in principle, a plethysmograph especially modified for use upon the kidney of the living animal. It is a kidney-shaped box of thin brass made in two parts, hinged at the back, and with a clasp in front to hold them together. In the interior of the box thin peritoneal membrane is so fastened to each half that a layer of olive oil may be placed between it and the brass walls. There is thus formed in each half a soft pad of oil upon which the kidney rests. When the kidney, freed as far as possible from fat and sur- rounding connective tissue, but with the blood-vessels and nerves entering at the hilus entirely uninjured, is laid in one-half of the on- cometer, and the other half is shut down upon it and tightly fas- tened, the organ is surrounded by oil in a box which is liquid-tight at every point except one, from which a tube is led off to some suitable recorder such as a tambour. Under these conditions every increase in the volume of the kidney causes a proportional outflow of oil from the oncometer, which is measured by the recorder, and every diminution in volume is accompanied by a reverse change. At the same time the flow of urine during these changes can be determined by inserting a cannula into the ureter and measuring * " Skandinavisches Archiv f. Physiologie, " 4, 241, 1892. 750 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. directly the outflow of urine. By this and other means it has been shown that the kidney receives a rich supply of vasoconstrictor nerve fibers that reach it between and around the entering blood- vessels. These fibers emerge from the spinal cord chiefly in the lower thoracic spinal nerves (tenth to thirteenth in the dog), pass through the sympathetic system, and reach the organ as postgan- glionic fibers. Stimulation of these nerves causes a contraction of the small arteries of the kidney, a shrinkage in volume of the whole organ as measured by the oncometer (see Fig. 223), and a dimin- ished secretion of urine. When, on the other hand, these con- strictor fibers are cut as they enter the hilus of the kidney, the ar- teries are dilated on account of the removal of the tonic action of the constrictor fibers, the organ enlarges, and a greater quantity of blood passes through it, since the resistance to the blood-flow is diminished while the general arterial pressure in the aorta remains practically the same. Along with this greater flow of blood there is a marked increase in the secretion of urine. Under normal conditions we must suppose that these fibers are brought into play to a greater or less extent by reflex stimulation, and thus serve to control the blood-flow through the kidney and thereby influence its functional activity. It has been shown, too, that the kidney receives vasodilator nerve-fibers, — that is, fibers which when stimulated directly or reflexly cause a dilatation of the arteries, and therefore a greater flow of blood through the or- gan. According to Bradford, these fibers emerge from the spinal cord mainly in the anterior roots of the eleventh, twelfth, and thir- teenth spinal nerves. Under normal conditions these fibers are probably thrown into action by reflex stimulation and lead to an increased functional activity. It will be seen, therefore, that the kidneys possess a local nervous mechanism through which their secretory activity may be increased or diminished by correspond- ing alterations in the blood-supply. So far is as known, this is the only way in which the secretion in the kidneys can be directly af- fected by the central nervous system. It should be borne in mind, also, that the blood-flow through the kidneys, and therefore their secretory activity, may be affected by conditions influencing general arterial pressure. Conditions such as asphyxia, strychnin poison- ing, or painful stimulation of sensory nerves, which cause a general vasoconstriction, influence the kidney in the same way, and tend, therefore, to diminish the flow of blood through it ; while conditions which lower general arterial pressure, such as general vascular dila- tation of the skin vessels, may also depress the secretory action of the kidney by diminishing the amount of blood flowing through it. In what way any given change in the vascular conditions of the body will influence the secretion of the kidney depends upon a num- KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 751 ber of factors and their relations to one another, but any change which will increase the difference in pressure between the blood in the renal artery and the renal vein will tend to augment the flow of blood unless it is antagonized by a simultaneous constriction in the small arteries of the kidney itself. On the contrary, any vas- cular dilatation of the vessels in the kidney will tend to increase the blood-flow through it unless there is at the same time such a general fall of blood-pressure as is sufficient to lower the pressure in the renal artery and reduce the driving force of the blood to an extent that more than counteracts the favorable influence of dimin- ished resistance in its small arteries. The Composition of Urine. — The urine of man is a yellowish liquid that varies greatly in depth of color. It has an average specific gravity of 1.020 and usually an acid reaction. This acid reaction is attributed generally to the presence of acid phosphates, particularly acid sodium phosphate (NaH2P04); but, according to Folin,* the acidity is due partially and indeed in larger part to or- ganic acids. When tested by the usual indicators (litmus) human urine may show an alkaline reaction, and, in fact, observations indicate that the reaction may vary in accordance with the character of the food. Among carnivora the urine is uniformly acid, and among herbivora it is alkaline so long as they use a veg- etable diet. During starvation, however, or when living upon the mothers' milk, — that is, whenever they are existing upon a purely animal diet — the urine becomes acid. The general explanation of this effect of food that has been suggested (Drechsel) is that upon an animal diet more -acids are formed (from the oxidation of the sulphur and phosphorus of the proteids) than in the case of the vegetable foods in which the alkaline salts of the vegetable acids give rise on oxidation in the body to alkaline carbonates. The kidney separates from the alkaline (neutral) blood and lymph the excess of salts and thus maintains a normal balance between the acid and basic equivalents in the blood. The composition of the urine is very complex. In addition to the water and inorganic salts the following elements are important, namely, urea, the purin bodies (uric acid, xanthin, hypoxanthin), creatinin, hippuric acid, oxalic acid (calcium oxalate), several conjugated sulphates and conjugated glycuronates, several aromatic oxyacids and nitrogenous acids, fatty acids, dissolved gases ( N and C02), and the urinary pigments urochrome and urobilin. This list is not complete; a number of additional substances have been de- scribed as occurring constantly or occasionally in traces within the limits of health. Under pathological conditions the composition may be still further modified. The complexity of the composition * " American Journal of Physiology, " 9, 265, 1903. 752 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. may be understood when it is recalled that through this organ are eliminated some of all the end-products formed in the various tis- sues, together with products arising from bacterial fermentation in the gastro-intestinal canal and various more or less foreign sub- stances taken with the food. It is not possible to describe all the numerous constituents that have been observed. Attention may be directed to those that quantitatively or otherwise are of chief physiological interest. The Nitrogen Elimination in the Urine. — Nearly all of the ex- cretion of nitrogen occurs in the urine. In the metabolism of the usual foodstuffs — carbohydrates, fats, and proteids — the end-prod- ucts of their destruction or physiological oxidation in the body are water, carbon dioxid, and nitrogenous waste products (and sulphates and phosphates from the sulphur and phosphorus in the proteids). The water is eliminated in the urine, the sweat, saliva, etc., and the expired air. The C02 is eliminated in the expired air, and in smaller part in dissolved form in the secretions (sweat, urine). The nitrog- enous excretion, representing the breaking down of proteid material, is found in minute part in the sweat, to a larger extent in the feces, but in by far the main amount in the urine. In all problems con- cerning proteid metabolism in the body, both as regards its char- acter and extent, the quantitative study of this excretion is of par- amount importance. In order to determine the total amount of proteid metabolism it is customary to determine the total nitrogen eliminated in the urine, without regard to its specific form. This determination is made usually by the method of Kjeldahl. The total weight of nitrogen multiplied by 6.25 gives the amount of pro- teid broken down, since nitrogen forms, on the average, 16 per cent, of the weight of the proteid molecule. In an average sized man the total nitrogen eliminated in a day varies, let us say, between 14 and 18 gms., which would correspond to 88 and 117 gms. of pro- teid. It being often necessary to distinguish between the forms in which this nitrogen is eliminated, the following distinctions are made: (1) The urea nitrogen, — that is, the nitrogen eliminated as urea. According to some recent analyses by Folin,* the urea nitrogen in man averages 87.5 per cent, of the total nitrogen. (2) The am- monia nitrogen, — that is, the nitrogen found in the form of am- monia salts which liberate free ammonia on the addition of a fixed alkali. The proportion of this ammonia nitrogen often varies, especially under pathological conditions affecting the liver. Its quantitative determination is a matter of importance. The aver- age amount in health may be stated (Folin) as 4.3 per cent, of the total nitrogen. (3) The creatinin nitrogen, — that is, the amount excreted as creatinin and indicative of a special (muscular) metab- * " American Physiological Journal, " 13, 45, 1905. KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 753 olism (3.6 per cent, of total nitrogen). (4) The purin body nitrogen (uric acid, xanthin, hypoxanthin), also indicative of a special metab- olism. Origin and Significance of Urea. — Urea has the formula, CO- N2H4. It may be considered as an amid of carbonic acid, and has, therefore, the structural formula of CO<^NH2. It occurs in the urine in relatively large quantities (2 per cent.). As the total quan- tity of urine secreted in twenty-four hours by an adult male may be placed at from 1500 to 1700 c.c, it follows that from 30 to 34 gms. of urea are eliminated from the body during this period. It is the most important of the nitrogenous excreta of the body, the chief end-product of the physiological oxidation of the proteids of the body, and also of the albuminoids when they appear in the food. If we know how much urea is secreted in a given period, we know approximately how much proteid has been broken down in the body in the same time. In round numbers, 1 gm. of proteid will yield J gm. of urea, as may be calculated easily from the amount of nitrogen contained in each. Since, however, some of the nitrogen of proteid is eliminated in other forms — uric acid, creatinin, etc. — even an exact determination of all the urea is not sufficient to de- termine with accuracy the total amount of proteid broken down. This fact is arrived at more perfectly, as stated above, by a deter- mination of the total nitrogen of the urine and other excretions. In addition to the urine, urea is found in slight quantities in other secretions, — in milk (in traces) and in sweat. In the latter liquid the quantity of urea in twenty-four hours may be quite appreciable, — as much, for instance, as 0.8 gm., — although such a large amount is found only after active exercise. It has been ascertained definitely that urea is not formed by the kidneys; it is brought to the kid- neys by the blood for elimination. That urea is not made in the kidneys is demonstrated by such facts as these: If blood, on the one hand, is irrigated through an isolated kidney, no urea is formed, even though substances (such as ammonium carbonate) from which urea is readily produced are added to the blood; on the other hand, urea is constantly present in the blood (0.0348 to 0.1529 per cent.), and if the two kidneys are removed, it continues to accumulate steadily in the blood as long as the animal survives. It has been ascertained that the urea is produced in part in the liver. The most important questions to be decided are: Through what steps is the proteid molecule metabolized to the form of urea? and, What is the antecedent substance brought to the liver, from which it makes urea? It is impossible to answer these questions perfectly, but recent investigations have thrown a great deal of light on the whole process, and they give hope that before long the entire history of 48 754 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. the derivation of urea from proteids and albuminoids will be known. The results of this work may be stated briefly as follows: 1 . Urea arises from proteids by a process of hydrolysis and oxida- tion, with the formation eventually of ammonia compounds, which are then conveyed to the liver and there changed to urea. Drechsel has suggested that ammonium carbamate forms one at least of the ammonia compounds that are converted to urea, and gives the fol- lowing evidence for this view. In the first place, Drechsel found carbamic acid in the blood of dogs, and Drechsel and Abel have shown that it occurs normally in the urine of horses as calcium car- bamate. Abel has shown, also, that it may be found in the urine of dogs or infants after the use of lime-water. Drechsel has shown, further, that ammonium carbamate may be converted into urea. If one compares the formulas of ammonium carbamate and urea, it is seen that the former may pass over into the latter by the loss of a molecule of water, as — Ammonium Urea, carbamate. Drechsel supposes, however, that this dehydration is effected in an indirect manner; that there is first an oxidation removing two atoms of hydrogen, and then a reduction removing an atom of oxy- gen. He succeeded in showing that when an aqueous solution of ammonium carbamate is submitted to electrolysis, and the direc- tion of the current is changed repeatedly so as to get alternately reduction and oxidation processes at each pole, some urea is pro- duced. These facts show the existence of ammonium carbamate in the body, and the possibility of its conversion to urea. It remains possible, however, that other salts or compounds of ammonia may likewise be converted normally to urea by the liver, since it has been shown experimentally in artificial circulation through this organ that salts such as ammonium carbonate, or even such complex ammonia compounds as leucin and glycocoll, may give rise to urea. Experiments made by Hahn, Pawlow, Massen, and Nencki* show that in dogs removal of the liver is followed by a decrease in the amount of urea in the urine and an increase in the ammonia con- tents. In these remarkable experiments a fistula (Eck fistula) was made between the portal vein and the inferior vena cava, the result of which was that the whole portal circulation of the liver was abolished, and the only blood that the organ received was through the hepatic artery. If, now, this artery was ligated or the liver was cut away, as was done in some of the experiments, then the result was practically an extirpation of the entire organ. The * " Archiv f. experimentelle Pathologie u. Pharmakologie, " 32, 161, 1893. KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 755 animals in these investigations survived this operation for some time, but they died finally, showing a series of symptoms which indicated a deep disturbance of the nervous system. It was found that the symptoms of poisoning in these animals could be brought on before they developed spontaneously by feeding the dogs upon a rich meat diet, or with salts of ammonia or carbamic acid. Later investiga- tions* showed that in normal animals the ammonia contents of the blood in the portal vein are from three to four times what is found in the arterial blood, but that after the operation described the ammonia in the arterial blood increases and at the time of the de- velopment of the fatal symptoms reaches about the percentage which is normal to the blood of the portal vein. It would seem from these investigations that the liver stands between the portal circulation and the general systemic circulation and protects the latter from the comparatively large amount of ammonia compounds contained in the portal blood by converting these compounds to urea. If the liver is thrown out of function, ammonia compounds accumulate in the blood and cause death. Similar ammonia salts are probably formed in other active proteid tissues, since the per- centage of ammonia in the tissues is considerably greater than in the blood, and these compounds also are doubtless converted to urea in the liver, in part at least. As to the origin of the ammonia compounds, there is little direct evidence. They come, in the long run, of course, from the nitrogenous foodstuffs, — proteids and al- buminoids. Drechsel supposes that the proteids first undergo hydro- lytic cleavage, with the formation of amido-bodies, such as leucin, tyrosin, aspartic acid, glycocoll, etc.; that these bodies undergo oxidation in the tissues, with the formation of NH3, C02, and H20; and that the NH2 and C03 then unite synthetically to form am- monium carbamate, which is carried to the liver and changed to urea. It is a very significant fact that the relative and absolute amount of urea nitrogen in the urine varies directly with the amount of proteid taken as food, while other nitrogenous constituents of the urine (creatinin, purin bases) are practically not affected by the food, if care is taken to have the food free of these substances to begin with. Folin has laid emphasis upon this fact,f and suggests, therefore, that most of the urea may come directly from proteid of the food which is hydrolyzed during digestion and absorption (action of trypsin and erepsin) into simpler amido-acids. These amido- bodies by further hydrolysis and oxidation may be converted, so far as their nitrogen is concerned, into ammonia compounds and eliminated at once as urea b}^ the liver without entering into tissue * Nencki, Pawlow, and Zaleski, ibid., 37, 26, 1895; also Nencki and Pawlow, "Archives des sciences biologiques, " 5, 213. t Folin, "American Journal of Physiology," 13, 117, 1905. 9 , ,, 756 * PHJfcSI^LOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. all. In this way the large proportion of ammonia compounds in the portal blood after digestion may be explained. Granting that a portion, perhaps a large portion, of the urea arises from this early hydrolysis of the proteid of the food, we must admit also at present that ammonia compounds may be formed in the tis- sues of the body generally, probably by a similar process of hydrol- ysis followed by oxidation. 2. It is stated (Kossel and Dakin*) that a ferment (arginase) may be extracted from the liver which is capable of splitting arginin into urea and ornithin (diamido valerianic acid). Since arginin is one of the diamido-bodies formed by the hydrolysis of the proteids during digestion, it is possible that some of the urea has this origin. The fact lends some probability to the view suggested above that much of the nitrogen of proteid food may be converted to urea before entering the general circulation. 3. Even after the removal of the liver some urea is still found in the urine. This fact proves that other organs have the power of forming urea, but what these other organs are and by what process they make urea are points as yet undecided. It seems as though the urea-forming power of the liver is shared by some of the other tissues, just as its glycogenic functions are. Origin and Significance of the Purin Bodies (Uric Acid, Xanthin, Hypoxanthin) . — These bodies are related chemically, and appear also to have a common physiological significance. Their chemical relations have been described by Emil Fischer, to whom we owe the term purin bodies. Fischer pointed out that these and other substances belonging to this group have a common nucleus: N — C C C — Nv which he named the purin nucleus. The I I > N — C — W hydrogen compound of this nucleus would be designated as purin, N = CH and would have the formula: HC C— NH , C5H4N4. Addi- l| II \PTT N — C — N^CH tion of an atom of oxygen gives hypoxanthin, C5H4N40: HN — CO HC C — NH 11 J! ^CH. Addition of two* atoms of oxygen gives xan- N — C — N^ HN — CO thin, C5H4N402: CO C — NH HN — C — N/°H' AnC* adcution °f tnree atoms * Kossel and Dakin, "Zeitschrift f. physiol. Chemie," 42, 181, 1904. R A F KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGAl HN — CO ^"<^U of oxygen gives uric acid, C5H4N403: CO C — NH , which HN — C — NH from this standpoint might be named trioxypurin. If one of the H atoms in the purin is substituted by an amido-group, NH2, the com- pound, adenin (C5H5N5), is obtained, and the further addition of oxy- gen gives guanin (C5H5N50). Moreover, caffein, the active principle of coffee and tea, and theobromin, the active principle of cocoa, are re- spectively trimethyl and dimethyl compounds of purin. Uric acid, xanthin, hypoxanthin, and in smaller amounts other members of this group are found constantly in the urine. It has been pointed out* that these substances come partly from purin bodies taken as food. If materials containing the purin bodies, such as meat, are fed these bodies are excreted in part in the urine. It is proposed to designate the uric acid, etc., that has this origin as the exogenous purin ma- terial. A portion of the amount daily secreted comes, however, from a metabolism of the proteid material of the body, and this portion may be distinguished as the endogenous purin bodies. This latter amount is found to be practically constant, 0.15 to 0.20 gm. per day for any one individual, and the amount is not affected by changes in the quantity or character of the food, but varies within certain limits with the manner of life. Evidently the endogenous purin nitrogen represents a special metabolism, probably of the liv- ing tissues, that goes on independently, in great measure, of the mere oxidation of food. The view generally adopted at present is that first proposed by Horbaczewsky,f — namely, that the purin bodies are the end-product, so far as the nitrogen is concerned, of the physiological oxidation of the nuclein (see appendix) found in the nuclei of the cells, and especially perhaps of the nuclein of the leucocytes. On this view the purin bodies give a measure of the extent of metabolism in the cell-nuclei. The actual amount of these substances found in the urine does not in all probability represent the entire amount formed in the body. It is found that when xanthin, hypoxanthin, uric acid, or materials such as liver or thymus rich in purin bodies are fed, only about half of the material is ex- creted as such in the urine, and it seems probable that the same fate occurs to a part of the endogenous purin material normally formed in the body. Among birds and reptiles uric acid represents the chief nitrogenous excretion of the urine, taking physiologically the place held by urea in the mammalia. In the birds it has been shown that the uric acid is formed in the liver. Extirpation of the kidneys in these animals leads to an accumulation of uric acid in * See Burian and Schur, " Archiv f . die gesammte Physioloeie, " 94, 273. 1903. t See Minkowski, "Archiv f. exper. Pathol, u. Pharmakol.," 41, 375. structural formula is given as NHC/ and its chemical re- 758 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. the blood and tissues, while removal of the liver, on the contrary, causes a decrease in the excretion of uric acid and an increase in the ammonia contents of the urine. It may be concluded, there- fore, that in birds uric acid is formed in part at least in the liver from ammonia compounds. Whether the liver takes a part in the formation of the endogenous uric acid in mammals has not been positively shown. Origin and Significance of the Creatinin. — Creatinin (C4H7- N30) is derived from the creatin (C4H9N302) found in muscle. Its /NH — CO v ^N(CH3 lations are indicated by the fact that it may be prepared synthetically from methyl-glycocoll and cyanamid, — that is, the union of these two substances gives creatin, from which in turn creatinin may be obtained. N=C-NH, + NH(CHJCHPOOH - NHC^H$)CHjC00H Cyanamid. Methyl-glycocoll. Creatin. Creatinin occurs in the urine constantly and in amounts equal to 1 to 2 gms. per day. Next to the urea and the ammonia com- pounds it forms the most important nitrogenous constituent of the urine. Its physiological history is imperfectly known. The fol- lowing facts, however, are significant and throw some light on its origin. Like the purin bodies, the amount present in the urine is probably partly of an exogenous and partly of an endogenous origin, — that is, part is formed in the body and part arises from the creatin contained in the meats and soups used as food. The endogenous portion, which, of course, is the part that is interesting physio- logically, shows a tendency to remain constant under constant con- tions of life, and this fact indicates (Folin) that the creatinin repre- sents an end-product of the metabolism of living or organized pro- teid tissue rather than one of the results of the metabolism of the food proteid. Everything would indicate also that this substance originates in the muscular tissue. Creatin is a constant and consider- able constituent of muscle, and a fair inference, therefore, is that it originates in this tissue from the catabolism of the muscle sub- stance, and is subsequently given to the blood and excreted as creat- inin. A difficulty in regard to this last hypothesis is found in the fact that the mass of muscular tissue in the body contains a relatively large amount of creatin (90 gms.) and yet only 1 to 2 gms. are excreted in the urine during the day. On account of this dis- proportion it has been suggested that some of the creatin may be converted to urea, but no proof has been furnished as yet that the body can accomplish this transformation. Creatin given in KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 759 the food is eliminated as creatinin. As is described in the section on Nutrition, it is known that increased muscular work may or may not increase the nitrogen output in the urine according to the diet used. Several observers have claimed that muscular activity in- creases the amount of creatinin in the urine,* but the increase is not so distinct nor so invariable that one may conclude satisfactorily that it is due to actual increase in production in the muscle. Others state that the increase is observable only after excessive muscular activity. Kochf has suggested, on account of the methyl groups present, that a part of the creatinin may arise from a metabolism of the lecithin. Hippuric Acid. — This substance has the formula C9H9N03. Its molecular structure is known, since upon decomposition it yields benzoic acid and glycocoll, and, moreover, it may be produced syn- thetically by the union of these two substances. Hippuric acid may be described, therefore, as a benzoyl-amido-acetic acid (CH2- NH[C6H5CO]COOH). It is found in considerable quantities in the urine of herbivorous animals (1.5 to 2.5 per cent.), and in much smaller amounts in the urine of man and of the carnivora. In human urine, on an average diet, about 0.7 gm. is excreted in twenty- four hours. If the diet is largely vegetable, this amount may be much increased. This last fact is readily explained, for it has been found that if benzoic acid or substances containing this grouping are fed to animals they appear in the urine as hippuric acid. Evi- dently a synthesis occurs in the body, and Bunge and Schmie- deberg proved conclusively that in dogs the union of benzoic acid and glycocoll to form hippuric acid takes place in the kidney itself. Later it was discovered J that the same synthesis may be effected by ground-up kidney tissue, mixed with blood and kept under oxygen pressure. It seems possible, therefore, that the synthesis is due to some specific constituent of the kidney cells, possibly an enzyme. Vegetable foods contain benzoic acid com- pounds, and we can understand, therefore, why when fed they in- crease the hippuric acid output of the urine. Since, however, in starving animals or animals fed upon meat hippuric acid is still present in the urine, although reduced in amount, it is evident that it arises in part as a result of the body metabolism. It should be added finally that some of the hippuric acid may be derived from the process of proteid putrefaction that occurs in the large intestine. The Conjugated Sulphates and the Sulphur Excretion. — The sulphur excretion of the urine possesses an importance similar * Gregor, " Zeitschrift f. physiol. Chemie, " 31, 98, 1900. t " American Journal of Physiology, " 13, xix, 1905. j Bashford and Cramer, "Zeitschrift f. physiolog. Chemie," 35, 324. 1902. 760 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. to that of nitrogen. Sulphur constitutes an element in most of the proteids, and in some form, therefore, it will be represented in the end-products of proteid metabolism. The sulphur elimination in the urine, like the nitrogen elimination, has been taken as a measure of the amount of proteid destruction. In the urine the sulphur occurs in three forms: (1) In an oxidized form as inorganic sulphates. Some of the sulphates are undoubtedly derived or may be derived from the mineral sulphates ingested with the food, but the larger part arises from the oxidation of the sulphur of the proteids. (2) The so-called conjugated or ethereal sulphates are combinations be- tween sulphuric acid and indoxyl, skatoxyl, phenol, and cresol, giving us phenolsulphuric acid (C6H5OS02OH), cresolsulphuric acid (C7H7- OS02OH), indoxylsulphuric acid or indican (C8H6NOS02OH), and skatoxylsulphuric acid (C9H8NOS02OH). The indol, skatol, phe- nol, and cresol are formed in the large intestine as a result of bac- terial putrefaction. They are eliminated in part in the feces, but in part are absorbed into the blood, and after oxidation are conjugated with sulphuric acid and eliminated in the urine. The process of conjugation is valuable from a physiological standpoint, as it converts substances having an injurious action into harmless compounds. It should be added, also, that to a small extent the phenol, indoxyl, and skatoxyl may be secreted in the urine as con- jugated glucuronates, — that is, in combination with glucuronic acid (C6H10O7), a reducing substance closely connected with dextrose. From a nutritional standpoint the amount of these substances pres- ent furnishes a measure of the extent of proteid putrefaction in the intestine, by virtue of the indol and phenol constituents. All con- ditions that increase the putrefactive processes in the intestine are accompanied by a parallel increase in the ethereal sulphates. By virtue of the sulphuric acid component these bodies represent also one of the forms in which sulphur is excreted from the body. (3) Some of the sulphur in the urine may occur in unoxid- ized form as sulphocyanid or as ethyl-sulphide (Abel) ([C2H5]2S). Under certain pathological conditions (cystinuria) some sulphur may be excreted in the form of cystin, but this is not a normal con- stituent of the urine. For other most interesting and significant changes in the composition of the urine under pathological condi- tions reference must be made to special works upon the urine or upon pathological chemistry. Water and Inorganic Salts. — Water is lost from the body through three main channels, — namely, the lungs, the skin, and the kidney, the last of these being the most important. The quan- tity of water lost through the lungs probably varies within small limits only. The quantity lost through the sweat varies, of course, with the temperature, with exercise, etc., and it may be said that KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 761 the amounts of water secreted through kidney and skin stand in something of an inverse proportion to each other; that is, the greater the quantity lost through the skin, the less will be secreted by the kidneys. Through these three organs, but mainly through the kidneys, the blood is being continually depleted of water, and the loss must be made up by the ingestion of new water. When water is swallowed in excess the superfluous amount is rapidly eliminated through the kidneys. The amount of water secreted may be in- creased by the action of diuretics, such as potassium nitrate and caffein. The inorganic salts of urine consist chiefly of the chlorids, phos- phates, and sulphates of the alkalies and the alkaline earths. It may be said, in general, that they arise partly from the salts ingested with the food, which salts are eliminated from the blood by the kid- ney in the water secretion, and in part they are formed in the de- structive metabolism that takes place in the body, particularly that involving the proteids and related bodies. Sodium chlorid occurs in the largest quantities, averaging about 15 gms. per day, of which the larger part, doubtless, is derived directly from the salt taken in the food. The phosphates occur in combination with cal- cium and magnesium, but chiefly as the acid phosphates of sodium or potassium. The acid reaction of the urine is usually attributed to these latter substances. The phosphates result in part from the destruction of phosphorus-containing tissues in the body, but chiefly from the phosphates of the food. The sulphates of urine are found partly in an oxidized form as simple sulphates or con- jugated with organic compounds, as described above. Micturition. — The urine is secreted continuously by the kid- neys, is carried to the bladder through the ureters, and is then at intervals finally ejected from the bladder through the urethra by the act of micturition. Movements of the Ureters. — The ureters possess a muscular coat consisting of an internal longitudinal and external circular layer. The contractions of this muscular coat are the means by which the urine is driven from the pelvis of the kidney into the bladder. The movements of the ureter have been carefully studied by Engel- mann.* According to his description, the musculature of the ureter contracts spontaneously at intervals of ten to twenty seconds (rab- bit), the contraction beginning at the kidney and progressing toward the bladder in the form of a peristaltic wave and with a velocity of about 20 to 30 mms. per second. The result of this movement should be the forcing of the urine into the bladder in a series of gentle, rhythmical spurts, and this method of filling the * " Pfliiger's Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 2, 243, 1869, and 4, 33. 762 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. bladder has been observed in the human being. Suter and Mayer* report some observations upon a boy in whom there was ectopia of the bladder, with exposure of the orifices of the ureters. The flow into the bladder was intermittent and was about equal upon the two sides for the time the child was under observation (three and a half days). The causation of the contractions of the ureter musculature is not easily explained. Engelmann finds that artificial stimulation of the ureter or of a piece of the ureter may start peristaltic con- tractions which move in both directions from the point stimulated. He was not able to find ganglion cells in the upper two-thirds of the ureter and was led to believe, therefore, that the contraction orig- inates in the muscular tissue independently of extrinsic or intrinsic nerves, and that the contraction wave propagates itself directly from muscle cell to muscle cell, the entire musculature behaving as though it were a single, colossal, hollow muscle fiber. The liber- ation of the stimulus which inaugurates the normal peristalsis of the ureter seems to be connected with the accumulation of urine in its upper or kidney portion. It may be supposed that the urine that collects at this point as it flows from the kidney stimulates the muscular tissue to contraction, either by its pressure or in some other way, and thus leads to an orderly sequence of contraction waves. It is possible, however, that the muscle of the ureter, like that of the heart, is spontaneously contractile under normal conditions, and does not depend upon the stimulation of the urine. Thus, according to Engelmann, section of the ureter near the kidney does not materially affect the nature of the contractions of the stump attached to the kidney, although in this case the pressure of the urine could scarcely act as a stimulus. Moreover, in the case of the rat, in which the ureter is highly contractile, the tube may be cut into several pieces and each piece will continue to exhibit period- ical peristaltic contractions. It does not seem possible at present to decide between these two views as to the cause of the contrac- tions. The nature of the contractions, their mode of progression, and the way in which they force the urine through the ureter seem, however, to be clearly established. Efforts to show a regulatory action upon these movements through the central nervous system have so far given negative results. Movements of the Bladder. — The bladder contains a muscular coat of plain muscle tissue, which, according to the usual description, is arranged so as to make an external longitudinal coat and an internal circular or oblique coat. A thin, longitudinal layer of muscle tissue lying to the interior of the circular coat is also de- scribed. The separation between the longitudinal and circular * "Archiv f. ex per. Pathologie und Pharmakologie," 32, 241, 1893. KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 763 layers is not so definite as in the case of the intestine; they seem, in fact, to form a continuous layer, one passing gradually into the other by a change in the direction of the fibers. At the cervix the circular layer is strengthened, and has been supposed to act as a sphincter with regard to the urethral orifice — the so-called sphinc- ter vesica? internus. Around the urethra just outside the blad- der is a circular layer of striated muscle that is frequently desig- nated as the external sphincter or sphincter urethral. The urine brought into the bladder accumulates within its cavity to a certain limit. It is prevented from escaping through the urethra at first by the mere elasticity of the parts at the urethral orifice, aided per- haps by tonic contraction of the internal sphincter, although this function of the circular layer is disputed by some observers. When the accumulation becomes greater the external sphincter is brought into action. If the desire to urinate is strong the external sphincter seems undoubtedly to be controlled by voluntary effort, but whether or not, in moderate filling of the bladder, it is brought into play by an involuntary reflex is not definitely determined. Backflow of urine from the bladder into the ureters is effectually prevented by the oblique course of the ureters through the wall of the blad- der. Owing to this circumstance, pressure within the bladder serves to close the mouths of the ureters, and, indeed, the more completely, the higher the pressure. At some point in the filling of the bladder the pressure is sufficient to arouse a conscious sen- sation of fullness and a desire to micturate. Under normal condi- tions the act of micturition follows. It consists essentially in a strong contraction of the bladder, with a simultaneous relaxation of the external sphincter, if this muscle is in action, the effect of which is to obliterate more or less completely the cavity of the blad- der and drive the urine out through the urethra. The force of this contraction is considerable, as is evidenced by the height to which the urine may spurt from the end of the urethra. According to Mosso, the contraction may support, in the dog, a column of liquid two meters high. The contractions of the blad- der may be and usually are assisted by contractions of the walls of the abdomen, especially toward the end of the act. As in defeca- tion and vomiting, the contraction of the abdominal muscles, when the glottis is closed so as to keep the diaphragm fixed, serves to in- crease the pressure in the abdominal and pelvic cavities, and thus assists in or 'completes the emptying of the bladder. It is, however, not an essential part of the act of micturition. The last portions of the urine escaping into the urethra are ejected, in the male, in spurts produced by the rhythmical contractions of the bulbocavernosus muscle. Considerable uncertainty and difference of opinion exists as to 764 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. the physiological mechanism by which this series of muscular con- tractions, and especially the contractions of the bladder itself, are produced. According to the frequently quoted description given by Goltz,* the series of events is as follows: The distention of the bladder by the urine causes finally a stimulation of the sensory fibers of the organ and produces a reflex contraction of the blad- der musculature which squeezes some urine into the urethra. The first drops, however, that enter the urethra stimulate the sensory nerves there and give rise to a conscious desire to urinate. If no obstacle is presented the bladder then empties itself, assisted per- haps by the contractions of the abdominal muscles. The emptying of the bladder may, however, be prevented, if desirable, by a volun- tary contraction of the sphincter urethrae, which opposes the effect of the contraction of the bladder. If the bladder is not too full and the sphincter is kept in action for some time, the contractions of the bladder may cease and the desire to micturate pass off. Ac- cording to this view, the voluntary control of the process is limited to the action of the external sphincter and the abdominal muscles; the contraction of the bladder itself is purely an unconscious reflex taking place through a lumbar center. The experiments of Goltz and others, upon dogs in which the spinal cord was severed at the junction of the lumbar and the tho- racic regions, indicate that micturition is essentially a reflex act, with its center in the lumbar cord, although the same observer has shown that in dogs whose spinal cord has been entirely destroyed, except in the cervical and upper thoracic region, the bladder emp- ties itself normally without the aid of external stimulation. Mosso and Pellacanit have made experiments upon women in which a catheter was introduced into the bladder and connected with a record- ing apparatus to measure the volume of the bladder. Their ex- periments indicate that the sensation of fullness and desire to micturate come from sensory stimulation in the bladder itself caused by the pressure of the urine. They point out that the bladder is very sensitive to reflex stimulation; that every psychical act and every sensory stimulus is apt to cause a contraction or in- creased tone of the bladder. The bladder is therefore subject to continual changes in size from reflex stimulation, and the pressure within it will depend not simply on the quantity of urine, but on the condition of tone of its muscles. At a certain pressure the sensory nerves are stimulated and under normal conditions mictu- rition ensues. We may understand, from this point of view, how it happens that we have sometimes a strong desire to micturate when the bladder contains but little urine, — for example, under emotional * " Archiv f . die gesammte Physiologie, " 8, 478, 1874. t" Archives italiennes de biologie, " 1, 1882. KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 765 excitement. In such cases if the micturition is prevented, probably by the action of the external sphincter, the bladder may sub- sequently relax and the sensation of fullness and desire to micturate pass away until the urine accumulates in sufficient quantity, or the pressure is again raised by some circumstance which causes a reflex contraction of the bladder. Nervous Mechanism. — According to Langley and Anderson,* the bladder in cats, dogs, and rabbits receives motor fibers from two sources: (1) From the lumbar nerves, the fibers passing out in the second to the fifth lumbar nerves and reaching the bladder through the sympathetic chain and the inferior mesenteric ganglion and hypogastric nerves. Stimulation of these nerves causes compara- tively feeble contraction of the bladder. (2) From the sacral spinal nerves, the fibers originating in the second and third sacral spinal nerves, or in the rabbit in the third and fourth, and taking their course through the so-called nervus erigens. Stimulation of these nerves, or some of them, causes strong contractions of the blad- der, sufficient to empty its contents. Little evidence was obtained of the presence of vasomotor fibers. According to Nawrocki and Skabitschewsky,f the spinal sensory fibers to the bladder are found in part in the posterior roots of the first, second, third, and fourth sacral spinal nerves, particularly the second and third. When these fibers are stimulated they excite reflexly the motor fibers to the bladder found in the anterior roots of the second and third sacral spinal nerves. Some sensory fibers to the bladder may pass by way of the hypogastric nerves. When the central stump of one hypogastric nerve is stimulated it produces, according to these authors, a reflex effect upon the motor fibers in the other hypo- gastric nerve, causing a - contraction of the bladder, the reflex oc- curring through the inferior mesenteric ganglion. This observa- tion has been confirmed by several authorities, but has been ex- plained by Langley and Anderson as a pseudoreflex or axon reflex (see p. 142). The immediate spinal center through which the contractions of the bladder may be reflexly stimulated or inhibited lies, accord- ing to the experiments of Goltz, in the lumbar portion of the cord, probably between the second and fifth lumbar spinal nerves. In dogs in which this portion of the cord was isolated by a cross-section at the junction of the thoracic and lumbar regions, micturition still ensued when the bladder was sufficiently full, and it could be called forth reflexly by sensory stimuli, especially by slight irritation of the anal region. This localization has been confirmed by others.J *" Journal of Physiology," 19, 71, 1895. t"Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 49, 141, 1891. % See Stewart, " American Journal of Physiology, " 2, 182, 1899. 766 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. Excretory Functions of the Skin. — The physiological activi- ties of the skin are varied. It forms, in the first place, a sensory surface covering the body, and interposed, as it were, between the external world and the inner mechanism. Nerve fibers of pressure, temperature, and pain are distributed over its surface, and by means of these fibers reflexes of various kinds are effected which keep the body adapted to changes in its environment. The physiology of the skin from this standpoint is discussed in the section on special senses. Again, the skin plays a part of immense value to the body in regulating the body temperature. This regulation, which is effected by variations in the blood supply or the sweat secretion, is described at appropriate places in the sections oh Nutrition and Circulation. In the female, during the period of lactation, the mam- mary glands, which must be reckoned amoM the organs of the skin, form an important secretion, the milkj^Be physiology of this gland is referred to in the section on ReprocMrction. In this section we are concerned with the physiology of me skin from a different standpoint, — namely, as an excretory organ. The excretions of the skin are formed in the sweat-glands and the sebaceous glands. Sweat. — The sweat or perspiration is a secretion of the sweat glands. These latter structures are found over the entire cutaneous surface except in the deeper portions of the external auditory meatus, the prepuce, and the glans penis. They are particularly abundant upon the palms of the hands and the soles of the feet. Krause estimates that their total number for the whole cutaneous surface is about two millions. In man they are formed on the type of simple tubular glands; the terminal portion contains the secretory cells, and at this part the tube is usually coiled to make a more or less compact knot, thus increasing the extent of the secreting sur- face. The larger ducts have a thin, muscular coat of involuntary tissue that may possibly be concerned in the ejection of the secre- tion. The secretory cells in the terminal portion are columnar in shape, possess a granular cytoplasm, and are arranged in a single layer. The amount of secretion formed by these glands varies greatly, being influenced by the condition of the atmosphere as re- gards temperature and moisture, as well as by various physical and psychical states, such as exercise and emotions. The average quan- tity for twenty-four hours is said to vary between 700 and 900 gms., although this amount may be doubled under certain conditions. According to an interesting paper by Schierbeck,* the average quantity of sweat in twenty-four hours may amount to 2 to 3 liters in a person clothed, and therefore with an average temperature of 32° C. surrounding the skin. This author states that the amount *"Archiv f. Physiologie, " 1893, 116; see also Willebrand, "Skandi- navisches Archiv f. Physiologie," 13, 337, 1902. KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 767 of sweat given off from the skin in the form of insensible perspira- tion increases proportionately with the temperature until a certain critical point is reached (about 33° C. in the person investigated), when there is a marked increase in the water eliminated, the in- crease being simultaneous with the formation of visible sweat. At the same time there is a sudden increase in the C02 eliminated from the skin. It is possible that the sudden increase in C02 is an in- dication of greater metabolism in the sweat glands in connection with the formation of visible sweat. Composition of the Secretion. — The precise chemical composition of sweat is difficult to determine, owing to the fact that as usually obtained it is liable to be mixed with the sebaceous secretion. Nor- mally it is a very thin secretion of low specific gravity (1.004) and an alkaline reaction, although when first secreted the reaction may be acid owing to admixture with the sebaceous material. The larger part of the inorganic salts consists of sodium chlorid. Small quantities of the alkaline sulphates and phosphates are also pres- ent. The organic constituents, though present in mere traces, are quite varied in number. Urea, uric acid, creatinin, aromatic oxy- acids, ethereal sulphates of phenol and skatol, and albumin, are said to occur when the sweating is profuse. Argutinsky has shown that after the action of vapor baths, and as the result of muscular work, the amount of urea eliminated in this secretion may be con- siderable. Under pathological conditions involving a diminished elimination of urea through the kidneys it has been observed that the amount found in the sweat is markedly increased, so that crys- tals of it may be deposited upon the skin. Under perfectly nor- mal conditions, however, it is obvious that the organic constituents are of minor importance. The main fact to be considered in the secretion of sweat is the formation of water. Secretory Fibers to the Sweat Glands. — Definite experimental proof of the existence of sweat nerves was first obtained by Goltz * in some experiments upon stimulation of the sciatic nerve in cats. In the cat and dog, in which sweat glands occur on the balls of the feet, the presence of sweat nerves may be demonstrated with great ease. . Electrical stimulation of the peripheral end of the divided sciatic nerve, if sufficiently strong, will cause visible drops of sweat to form on the hairless skin of the balls of the feet. When the electrodes are kept at the same spot on the nerve and the stimula- tion is maintained the secretion soon ceases; but this effect seems to be due to a temporary injury of some kind to the nerve fibers at the point of stimulation, and not to a genuine fatigue of the sweat glands or the sweat fibers, since moving the electrodes to a new point on the nerve farther toward the periphery calls forth a * " Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 11, 71, 1875. 768 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. new secretion. The secretion so formed is thin and limpid, and has a marked alkaline reaction. The anatomical course of these fibers has been worked out in the cat with great care by Langley.* He finds that for the hind feet they leave the spinal cord chiefly in the first and second lumbar nerves, enter the sympathetic chain, and emerge from this as postganglionic fibers in the gray rami, proceed- ing from the sixth lumbar to the second sacral ganglion, but chiefly in the seventh lumbar and first sacral, and then join the nerves of the sciatic plexus. For the forefeet the fibers leave the spinal cord in the fourth to the tenth thoracic nerves, enter the sympathetic chain, pass upward to the first thoracic ganglion, whence they are continued as postganglionic fibers that pass out of this ganglion by the gray rami communicating with the nerves forming the brachial plexus. The action of the nerve fibers upon the sweat glands can not be explained as an indirect effect, — for instance, as a result of a variation in the blood-flow. Experiments have repeatedly shown that, in the cat, stimulation of the sciatic still calls forth a secre- tion after the blood has been shut off from the leg by ligation of the aorta, or indeed after the leg has been amputated for as long as twenty minutes. So in human beings it is known that profuse sweating may often accompany a pallid skin, as in terror or nausea, while, on the other hand, the flushed skin of fever is char- acterized by the absence of perspiration. There seems to be no doubt that the sweat nerves are genuine secretory fibers, causing a secretion in consequence of a direct action on the cells of the sweat glands. In accordance with this physiological fact histological work has demonstrated that special nerve fibers are supplied to the glandular epithelium. According to Arnstein, the terminal fibers form a small, branching, varicose ending in contact with the epithelial cells. The sweat gland may be made to secrete in many ways other than by direct artificial excitation of the sweat fibers, — for example, by external heat, dyspnea, muscular exercise, strong emotions, and by the action of various drugs, such as pilocarpin, muscarin, strychnin, nicotin, picrotoxin, and physostigmin. In all such cases the effect is supposed to result from an action on the sweat fibers, either directly on their terminations or indirectly upon their cells of origin in the central nervous system. In ordinary life the usual cause of profuse sweating is a high external temper- ature or muscular exercise. With regard to the former it is known that the high temperature does not excite the sweat glands im- mediately, but through the intervention of the central nervous system. If the nerves going to a limb be cut, exposure of that limb to a high temperature does not cause a secretion, showing that the temperature change alone is not sufficient to excite the *" Journal of Physiology," 12, 347, 1891. KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 769 gland or its terminal nerve fibers. We must suppose, therefore, that the high temperature acts upon the sensory cutaneous nerves, possibly the heat fibers, and reflexly stimulates the sweat fibers. Although external temperature does not directly excite the glands, it should be stated that it affects their irritability either by direct action on the gland cells or upon the terminal nerve fibers. At a sufficiently low temperature the cat's paw does not secrete at all, and the irritability of th'e glands is increased by a rise of temper- ature up to about 45° C. Dyspnea, muscular exercise, emotions, and many drugs affect the secretion, probably by action on the nerve centers. Pilocarpin, on the contrary, is known to stimulate the endings of the nerve fibers in the glands, while atropin has the opposite effect, com- pletely paralyzing the secretory fibers. Sweat Centers in the Central Nervous System. — The fact that secretion of sweat may be occasioned by stimulation of afferent nerves or by direct action upon the central nervous system, as in the case of dyspnea, implies the existence of physiological centers controlling the secretory fibers. The precise location of the sweat center or centers has not, however, been satisfactorily determined. Histologically and anatomically the arrangement of the sweat fibers resembles that of the vasoconstrictor fibers, and, reasoning from analogy, one might suppose the existence of a general sweat center in the medulla comparable to the vasoconstrictor center, but positive evidence of the existence of such an arrangement is lacking. It has been shown that when the medulla is separated from the cord by a section in the cervical or thoracic region the action of dyspnea, or of various sudorific drugs supposed to act on the central nervous system, may still cause a secretion. On the evidence of results of this character it is assumed that there are spinal sweat centers; but whether these are few in number or represent simply the various nuclei of origin of the fibers to different regions is not definitely known. It is possible that in addition to these spinal centers there is a general regulating center in the medulla. Sebaceous Secretion. — The sebaceous glands are simple or compound alveolar glands found over the cutaneous surface, usually in association with the hairs, although in some cases they occur separately, as, for instance, on the prepuce and glans penis, and on the lips. When they occur with the hairs the short duct opens into the hair follicle, so that the secretion is passed out upon the hair near the point at which it projects from the skin. The alveoli are filled with cuboidal or polygonal epithelial cells, which are arranged in several layers. Those nearest the lumen of the gland are filled with fatty material. These cells are supposed to be cast off bodily, their detritus going to form the secretion. New cells are formed 49 770 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. from the layer nearest the basement membrane, and thus the glands continue to produce a slow but continuous secretion. The sebaceous secretion, or sebum, is an oily, semiliquid material that sets, upon exposure to the air, to a cheesy mass, as is seen in the comedones or pimples which so frequently occur upon the skin from occlusion of the opening of the ducts. The exact composition of the secretion is not known. It contains fats and soaps, some cholesterin, albu- minous material (part of which is a nucleo-albumin often described as a casein), remnants of epithelial cells, and inorganic salts. The cholesterin occurs in combination with a fatty acid, and is found in especially large quantities in sheep's wool, from which it is extracted and used commercially under the name of lanolin. The sebaceous secretion from different places, or in different animals, is probably somewhat variable in composition as well as in quantity. The secretion of the prepuce is known as the smegma prceputii; that of the external auditory meatus, mixed with the secretion of the neigh- boring sweat glands or ceruminous glands, forms the well-known earwax or cerumen. The secretion in this place contains a reddish pigment of a bitterish-sweet taste, the composition of which has not been investigated. Upon the skin of the newly born the se- baceous material is accumulated to form the vernix caseosa. The well-known uropygal gland of birds is homologous with the mam- malian sebaceous glands, and its secretion has been obtained in sufficient quantities for chemical analysis. Physiologically it is believed that the sebaceous secretion affords a protection to the skin and hairs. Its oily character doubtless serves to protect the hairs from becoming too brittle, or, on the other hand, from being too easily saturated with external moisture. In this way it prob- ably aids in making the hairy coat a more perfect protection against the effect of external changes of temperature. Upon the surface of the skin, also, it forms a thin, protective layer that tends to prevent undue loss of heat from evaporation, and possibly is important in other ways in maintaining the physiological integrity of the external surface. Excretion of C02. — In some of the lower animals — the frog, for example — the skin takes an important part in the respiratory exchanges, eliminating C02 and absorbing O. In man, and pre- sumably in the mammalia generally, it has been ascertained that changes of this kind are very slight. Estimates of the amount of CO 2 given off from the skin of man during twenty-four hours vary greatly, but the amount is small, about 7 to 8 gms. in twenty-four hours, unless there is marked sweating, in which case the amount is noticeably increased. CHAPTER XLVI. SECRETION OF THE DUCTLESS GLANDS-INTERNAL SECRETION* The term "internal secretion" is used to designate those secre- tions of glandular tissues which, instead of being carried off to the exterior by a duct, are eliminated in the blood or lymph. The idea that secretory products may be given off in this way has long been held in reference to the ductless glands, such as the thyroid, pitui- tary body, etc., the absence of a duct suggesting naturally such a possibility. The term, however, seems to have been employed first by Claude Bernard, who emphasized the distinction between the ordinary secretions, or external secretions, and this group of internal secretions. Modern interest in the latter is due largely to work done by Brown-Sequard (1889) upon testicular extracts, work which itself was of doubtful value. This author was led to amplify the conception of an internal secretion by the assumption that all tissues give off a something to the blood which is characteristic, and is of importance in general nutrition. This idea led in turn to a revival of some old notions regarding the treatment of diseases of the different organs by extracts of the corresponding tissue, a therapeutical method usually designated as opotherapy. Brown- Sequard's extension of the idea of internal secretion has not been justified by subsequent work, and to-day we must limit the term to definitely glandular tissues. Experience has shown, however, that not only the ductless glands, but some at least of the typical glands provided with ducts may give rise to internal secretions, the pancreas, for example, or the liver. In some of the ductless glands, on the contrary, the existence or non-existence of an internal secretion is still an open question. The work done since 1889 has, however, demonstrated fully that some of the ductless glands play a role of the very greatest importance in general nutrition, and this knowledge has proved useful in widening our conception of the nutritional relations in the organism and besides has found a valuable application in practical medicine. Liver. — We do not usually regard the liver as furnishing an internal secretion. As a matter of fact, it does form two products within its cells, — glycogen (sugar) and urea, which are subsequently given off to the blood for purposes of general nutrition or for elim- 771 772 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. ination. The processes in this case fall under the general defini- tion of internal secretion, and, in fact, may be used to illustrate specifically the meaning of this term. The history of glycogen and urea has been considered. Internal Secretion of the Thyroid Tissues. — The most im- portant and definite outcome of the work on internal secretions has been obtained with the thyroids. Recent experimental work on this organ makes it necessary for us now to distinguish between the thyroid and the parathyroid tissues. The thyroids proper form two oval bodies lying on the sides of the trachea at its junction with the larynx. They have no ducts, and are composed of vesicles of different sizes, which are lined by a single layer of cuboidal epithe- lium and contain in their interior a material known as colloid. A number of histologists have traced the formation of this colloid to the lining epithelial cells, and have stated, moreover, that the vesicles finally rupture and discharge the colloid into the surrounding lym- phatic spaces. Accessory thyroids varying in size and number may be found along the trachea as far down as the heart. They possess a vesicular structure and no doubt have a function similar to that of the thyroid body. The parathyroids are quite different structures. Four of these bodies are usually described, two on each side, and their positions vary somewhat in different animals. In man the superior (or internal) parathyroids . are found upon the posterior surface of the thyroid at the level of the junction of its upper with its middle third. This portion of the organ may be imbedded in the thyroid. The inferior (or external) parathyroids lie near the lower margin of the thyroid on its posterior surface, and in some cases lower down on the sides of the trachea. The tissue has a structure quite different from that of the thyroids, being composed of solid masses or columns of epithelial cells which are not arranged in vesicles and contain no colloid. Extirpation of the Thyroids and Parathyroids. — In 1856 Schiff showed that extirpation of the thyroids (complete thyroi- dectomy) in dogs is followed usually by the death of the animal in one to four weeks. The animal exhibits certain characteristic symp- toms, such as muscular tremors, which may pass into convulsions, cachexia, emaciation, and a condition of apathy. This result was confirmed by subsequent observers, but many exceptions were noted. Great interest was shown in these results, because on the surgical side reports were made showing that after complete removal of the thyroids in cases of goiter evil consequences might ensue, either acute convulsive attacks or chonic malnutrition. On the other hand, it became known that atrophy of the thyroids in the young is responsible for the condition of arrested growth and deficient SECRETION OF THE DUCTLESS GLANDS. 773 mental development designated as cretinism, and in the adult the same cause gives rise to the peculiar disease of myxedema, character- ized by distressing mental deterioration, an edematous condition of the skin, loss of hair, etc. Schiff and others found that the evil results of complete thyroidectomy in dogs might be obviated by grafting pieces of the thyroid in the body, and this knowledge was quickly applied to human beings in cases of myxedema and cretinism with astonishingly successful results. Instead of grafting thyroid tissue it was found, in fact, that injection of extracts under the skin or better still simple feeding of thyroid material gave similar favorable results: the individuals recovered their normal appear- ance and mental powers.* Later Baumannf succeeded in isolating from the glands a substance designated as iodothyrin, which shows in large measure the beneficial influence exerted by thyroid extracts in cases of myxedema and parenchymatous goiter. This substance is characterized by containing a large amount of iodin (9.3 per cent, of the dry weight). It is contained in the gland in combina- tion with proteid bodies, from which it may be separated by diges- tion with gastric juice or by boiling with acids. The Function of the Parathyroids. — Most of the results des- cribed above were obtained before the existence of the parathy- roids was recognized. Early in the history of the subject it was recognized that complete removal of the thyroids proper in herbiv- orous animals (rats, rabbits) is not attended by a fatal result. Gley and others, however, proved that if the parathyroids also are removed these animals die with the symptoms described in the case of dogs, cats, and other carnivorous animals. This result at- tracted attention to the parathyroids. Numerous experiments, especially by Moussu,t Gley,§ and Vassale and Generate, || have seemed to show a marked difference between the results of thyroi- dectomy and parathyroidectomy. When the parathyroids alone are removed the animal dies quickly with acute symptoms, muscular convulsions (tetany), etc.; when the thyroids alone are removed the animal may survive for a long period, but develops a condition of chronic malnutrition, — a slowly increasing cachexia which may exhibit itself in a condition resembling myxedema in man. This distinction has been generally accepted, and it throws much light upon the discrepancy in the results obtained by some of the earlier observers. Complete thyroidectomy with the acutely fatal results * For a general account of the development of the subject and the liter- ature see " Transactions of the Congress of American Physicians and Surgeons" (Howell, Chittenden, Adami, Putnam, Kinnicutt, Osier), 1897, and Jean- delize, " Insuffisance thyroidienne et parathyroidienne, " Nancy, 1902. t "Zeitschrift f. physiolog. Chemie," 21, 319, and 481, 1896. % Moussu, " Proc. Fourth International Physiolog. Congress, " 1898. §Gley, "Pfliiger's Archiv," 66, 308, 1897. || Vassale and Generate, " Archives italiennes de biologie, " 33, 1900. 774 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. usually described includes those cases in which both thyroids and parathyroids were removed, while probably many of the apparently negative results obtained after excision of the thyroids are expli- cable on the supposition that one or more of the parathyroids were left in the animal. It should be stated, however, that two recent observers, Vincent and Jolly,* as the result of numerous experi- ments made upon different varieties of animals, throw some doubt upon these conclusions. They contend that in herbivorous animals fully half of those operated upon survive complete removal of all thyroid tissue, showing no evil symptoms except perhaps a di- minished resistance to infection. Carnivorous animals, on the con- trary, usually die after such an operation. Assuming, however, that the distinction made by Moussu and others is well founded, the interesting question arises as to the functional relationship be- tween the thyroids and the parathyroids. Myxedema, cretinism, and similar conditions in man may be referred to an atrophy or loss of functions of the thyroids, but there is no knowledge of effects attributable to pathological changes in the parathyroids. The old view that the parathyroids represent embryonic thyroid tissue which after removal of the thyroids replaces the functions of the latter is not supported by experimental or histological evidence. The two bodies represent different structures, histologically, embryo- logically, and physiologically, and yet there seems to be some cor- relation in function between them. The parathyroids, like the thy- roids, contain a large percentage of iodin; when the parathyroids are removed the thyroids exhibit a change in structure in that the colloid material disappears from the vesicles, and finally the tetany following removal of the parathyroids may be ameliorated by sub- sequent excision of the thyroids. The exact nature of the functional connection between these two organs is, however, as yet quite un- explained; there is need for further investigation. The General Nature of the Functions of the Thyroids and Parathyroids. — Disregarding the difference in function between these two bodies, it is quite evident from the facts given that they exercise an important control over the processes of nutrition of the body, and especially perhaps over those of the central nervous system. How is this control exerted? Two general points of view have been advocated. According to one theory, the thyroid tis- sues elaborate a special internal secretion, characterized by its contents in iodin. This secretion is given off to the lymph or blood, is carried to the tissues, and there exercises a regulating action of an important or indeed essential character. Excision or atrophy of these bodies results in a loss of this secretion and a consequent malnutrition or perverted metabolism in other tissues of the organ- * Vincent and Jolly, " Journal of Physiology, " 32, 65, 1904. SECRETION OF THE DUCTLESS GLANDS. 775 ism. According to the other point of view, less generally held, the function of these bodies is to neutralize or destroy toxic substances formed in the metabolism of the rest of the body, as the liver, for instance, destroys the toxic character of the ammonia compounds by converting them to urea. On this theory the removal of the thyroid tissues results in the accumulation of toxic substances in the blood and the animal dies by a process of auto-intoxication. Cyon's View of the Function of the Thyroid. — Cyon in numerous publications has advocated a different view of the function of the thyroids. These bodies have a very large vascular supply, and this author assumes that this area serves as a vascular shunt or flood-gate to protect mechanically the circulation in the brain. The dilatation of the thyroid area under con- ditions that threaten congestion of the brain is effected reflexly by means of the hypophysis cerebri and the vagi. For details of this mechanism and also of the supposed effect of the thyroid secretion on the irritability of the centers innervating the heart and blood-vessels see " Archives de physiologie, " 1898, p. 618. Adrenal Bodies. — The adrenal bodies — or, as they are frequently called in human anatomy, the suprarenal capsules — belong to the group of ductless glands. It was shown first by Brown-Sequard (1856) that removal of these bodies is followed rapidly by death. This result has been confirmed by many experimenters, and so far as the observations go the effect of complete removal is the same in all animals. The fatal effect is more rapid than in the case of removal of the thyroids, death following the operation usually in two to three days, or, according to some accounts, within a few hours. The symptoms preceding death are great prostration, mus- cular weakness, and marked diminution in vascular tone. These symptoms resemble those occurring in Addison's disease in man, — a disease which clinical evidence has shown to be associated with pathological lesions in the suprarenal capsules. It has been ex- pected, therefore, that the results obtained from thyroid treatment of myxedema might be paralleled in cases of Addison's disease by the use of adrenal extracts, but so far these expectations have not been completely realized. Oliver* and Schaefer, and, about the same time, Cybulski and Szymonowicz,| discovered that this organ forms a peculiar substance that has a very definite physiological action, especially upon the circulatory system. They found that aqueous extracts of the medulla of the gland when injected into the blood of a living animal have a remarkable influence upon the heart and blood-vessels. If the vagi are intact, the adrenal extracts cause a very marked slowing of the heart beat together with a rise of blood- pressure. When the inhibitory fibers of the vagus are thrown out of action by section or by the use of atropin the heart rate is ac- * " Journal of Physiology, " 18, 230, 1895. t " Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 64, 97, 1896. 776 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. celerated, while the blood-pressure is increased sometimes to an extraordinary extent. These results are obtained with very small doses of the extracts. Schaefer states that as little as 5^ mgms. of the dried gland may produce a maximal effect upon a dog weigh- ing 10 kgms. The effects produced by such extracts are quite tem- porary in character. In the course of a few minutes the blood- pressure returns to normal, as also the heart beat, showing that the substance has been destroyed in some way in the body, although where or how this destruction occurs is not known. According to Schaefer, the kidneys and the adrenals themselves are not respon- sible for this prompt elimination or destruction of the active sub- stance. Several observers* have shown satisfactorily that the ma- terial producing this effect is present in perceptible quantities in the blood of the adrenal vein, so that there can be but little doubt that it is a distinct internal secretion of the adrenal. Dreyer has shown, moreover, that the amount of this substance in the adrenal blood is increased, judging from the physiological effects of its in- jection, by stimulation of the splanchnic nerve. Since this result was obtained independently of the amount of blood-flow through the gland, Dreyer makes the justifiable assumption that the adrenals possess secretory nerve fibers. Abelf has succeeded in isolating a substance from the gland that produces the effect on blood-pressure and heart rate, and proposes for it the name epinephrin hydrate. He assigns to it the formula C10H13NO3. iH20 and describes it as a peculiar, unstable, basic body. Salts of epinephrin may be ob- tained which when injected into the circulation cause the typical effects produced by injection of extracts of the gland. Other crystalline products, adrenalin (C9H13N03), suprarenalin, etc., have been prepared from the gland and show a most marked influence in constricting the blood-vessels. These substances are much used practically in minor surgical operations as a hemostatic to check the flow of blood. The constriction of the blood-vessels seems to be due to a direct effect upon the Walls of the vessels, either upon the musculature itself or upon the peripheral nerve fibers dis- tributed to these muscles. That the effect is not entirely central is indicated by the fact that after destruction of the vasoconstrictor center and removal of the spinal cord injection of the extracts causes a rise of blood-pressure of 100 per cent, or more. Langley has called attention to the peculiar fact that adrenal extracts or so- lutions of adrenalin do not act upon all varieties of plain muscle, but only on those innervated by nerve fibers originating in the sym- pathetic chain of ganglia. It has been proposed, therefore, to use these solutions to determine whether or not given vascular areas, * " American Journal of Physiology, " 2, 203, 1899. f Abel, " Berichte d. deut. chem. Gesellschaft, " 37, 368, 1904. SECRETION OF THE DUCTLESS GLANDS. 777 such as those of the brain or lungs, are provided with vasocon- strictor nerve fibers. When adrenalin is injected into a normal animal it may have an influence upon the nerve centers of the vaso- motor nerves as well as upon the peripheral endings. Meltzer* has shown that moderate doses under such conditions may cause a dila- tation, while in parts whose connection with the nerve center is destroyed only a constriction is obtained. Under normal condi- tions at least these extracts when injected into the circulation soon lose their effect. This fact may explain why injection of the ex- tracts has failed to give permanent relief in animals from whom the adrenals had been removed or in human beings suffering from Addison's disease. Bearing in mind the results obtained in the case of thyroidectomy, it has been suggested that grafts of the adre- nals under the skin or into the peritoneal cavity may prove more effective. Results by this method, however, have also been chiefly negative, owing apparently to the fact that in such grafts the medul- lary substance, which contains the material that causes constric- tion of the blood-vessels, readily undergoes atrophy and absorp- tion. It seems probable that, if the method of grafting is perfected so as to preserve the medulla intact, this procedure may prove effective as a therapeutical means in the treatment of Addison's disease. The Physiological Rdle of the Adrenals. — There seems to be no question that the medullary substance forms epinephrin or adre- nalin or some related compound which has a marked stimulating effect upon the tone of the blood-vessels and upon the heart, and that this material passes into the blood. The general view, there- fore, has been that one at least of the functions of the adrenals is the internal secretion of this material. It is assumed that its con- tinued formation is necessary to the maintenance of the normal metabolism of the muscular tissues either by a direct effect or indirectly by influencing the activity of the nerve centers. Removal of this secretion results in a marked loss of muscular tone and vigor, exhibited by the blood-vessels, the heart, and the skeletal muscles, and death follows rapidly. This general view is in accord with the facts so far as they are known, but it must be confessed that it goes somewhat beyond the facts. Another permissible, although less probable view is that the adrenals produce an antitoxic substance whose function is to neutralize or destroy certain (unknown) poi- sonous products of body metabolism. Removal or disease of the adrenals, on this theory, causes death because it allows these toxic products to accumulate. Pituitary Body. — This body is usually described as consisting *S. J. and Clara Meltzer, "American Journal of Physiology," 9, 252, 1903. 778 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. of two parts, — a large anterior lobe of distinct glandular structure and a much smaller posterior lobe whose structure is not clearly known. The cells are said to form follicles which contain some colloid material.* Embryologically the two lobes are entirely dis- tinct. The anterior lobe, which may be designated as the hypoph- ysis cerebri, arises from the epithelium of the mouth, while the posterior lobe, or the infundibular body, develops as an outgrowth from the infundibulum of the brain, and in the adult remains con- nected with this portion of the brain by a long stalk. Howell f and others have shown that extracts of the hypophysis when injected intravenously have little or no physiological effect, while extracts of the infundibular body, on the contrary, cause a marked rise of blood-pressure and slowing of the heart beat. These effects resemble in general those obtained from adrenal extracts, but differ in some details. They seem to warrant the conclusion that the infundib- ular body is not a mere rudimentary organ, as has been generally assumed, but produces a peculiar substance, an internal secretion, that may have a distinct physiological value. A number of ob- servers, especially Vassale and Sacchi, have succeeded in remov- ing the entire pituitary body. They report that the operation results eventually in the death of the animal with a certain group of symptoms, such as muscular tremors and spasms, apathy and dyspnea, that resemble the results of thyroidectomy. It has been suggested, therefore, that the pituitary body may be related in function to the thyroids and may be able to assume vicariously the functions of the latter after thyroidectomy. There is no satis- factory evidence, however, in support of this view. On the patho- logical side it has been shown that usually lesions of the pituitary body, particularly of the hypophysis, are associated with a peculiar disease known as acromegaly, the most prominent symptom of which is a marked hypertrophy of the bones of the extremities and of the face. The conclusion sometimes drawn from this fact that acromegaly is caused by a disturbance of the functions of the pitu- itary body is, however, very uncertain, and is not supported by any definite clinical or experimental facts. Testis and Ovary. — Some of the earliest work upon the effect of the internal secretions of the glands was done upon the repro- ductive glands, especially the testis, by Brown-Sequard.t Ac- cording to this observer, extracts of the fresh testis when injected under the skin or into the blood may have a remarkable influence upon the nervous system. Mental and physical vigor, and the * Thorn, "Archiv f. mik. Anat.," 57, 632, 1901. f "Journal of Experimental Medicine," 3, 245, -1898; also Schaefer and Vincent, " Journal of Physiology, " 25, 87, 1899. % " Archives de physiologie normale et pathologique, " 1889-92. SECRETION OF THE DUCTLESS GLANDS. 779 activity of the spinal centers, are greatly improved, not only in cases of general prostration and neurasthenia, but also in the case of the aged. Brown-Sequard maintained that this general dynamogenic effect is due to some unknown substance formed in the testis and subsequently passed into the blood, although he admitted that some of the same substance may be found in the ex- ternal secretion of the testis — i. e., the spermatic liquid. Poehl* asserts that he has prepared a substance, spermin, to which he gives the formula C5HUN2, which has a very beneficial effect upon the metabolism of the body. He believes that this spermin is the sub- stance that gives to the testicular extracts prepared by Brown-Se- quard their stimulating effect. He claims for this substance an extraordinary action as a physiological tonic. Zothf and also PregelJ seem to have obtained exact objective proof, by means of ergographic records, of the stimulating action of the testicular extracts upon the neuromuscular apparatus in man. They find that injections of the testicular extracts cause not only a diminu- tion in the muscular and nervous fatigue resulting from muscular work, but also lessen the subjective fatigue sensations. The fact that the internal secretion of the testis, if it exists at all, is not ab- solutely essential to the life of the body as a whole, as in the case of the thyroids, adrenals, and pancreas, naturally makes the satis- factory determination of its existence and action a more difficult task. Similar ideas in general prevail as to the possibility of the ovaries furnishing an internal secretion that plays an important part in general nutrition. In gynecological practice it has been observed that complete ovariotomy with its resulting premature menopause is often followed by distressing symptoms, mental and physical. In such cases many observers have reported that these symptoms may be alleviated by the use of ovarian extracts. Morris \ reports a number of cases in which, after complete removal of the ovaries, a piece of ovary from the same or a different person was grafted into the fundus of the uterus or into the broad ligament. In all cases menstruation persisted, showing, therefore, that the presence of the ovaries is necessary for this function. A similar operation in cases of amenorrhea or dysmenorrhea brought on free and easy menstruation and an improvement in general nutrition and well- being. Glass || also reports a case in which the entire ovary from one woman was transplanted into another patient upon whom com- plete ovariotomy had been performed two years before. The result * "Zeitschrift f. klinische Medicin," 26, 133, 1894. t"Pfluger's Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie," 62, 335, 1896; also 69, 386, 1897. % Ibid., p. 379. § Morris, " Medical Record," 1901, p. 83. || Glass, " Medical News," 1899, p. 523. 780 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. of the operation was a return of menstruation and sexual desire, and a marked alleviation of the disagreeable symptoms following the artificial menopause. In the natural menopause, as well as in the premature menopause following operations, it is a frequent, though not invariable result for the individual to gain noticeably in weight. The probability of an effect of the ovaries on general nutrition is indicated also by the interesting fact that in cases of osteomalacia, a disease characterized by softening of the bones, removal of the ovaries may exert a favorable influence upon the course of the disease. These indications have found some experimental verifi- cation in a research by Loewy and Richter* made upon dogs. These observers found that complete removal of the ovaries, al- though at first apparently without effect, resulted in the course of two to three months in a marked diminution in the consumption of oxygen by the animal, measured per kilogram of body-weight. If now the animal in this condition was given ovarian extracts (oophorin tablets), the amount of oxygen consumed was not only brought to its former amount, but considerably increased beyond it. A similar result was obtained when the extracts were used upon castrated males. The authors believe that their experiments show that the ovaries form a specific substance which is capable of increas- ing the oxidations of the body. Pancreas. — The importance of the external secretion, the pan- creatic juice, of the pancreas has long been recognized, but it was not until 1889 that von Mering and Minkowski f proved that it fur- nishes also an equally important internal secretion. These observers succeeded in extirpating the entire pancreas without causing the immediate death of the animal, and found that in all cases this operation was followed by the appearance of sugar in the urine in considerable quantities. Further observations of their own and other experimenters have corroborated this result and added a num- ber of interesting facts to our knowledge of this side of the activity of the pancreas. It has been shown that when the pancreas is com- pletely removed a condition of glycosuria inevitably follows, even if carbohydrate food is excluded from the diet. Moreover, as in the similar pathological condition of glycosuria or diabetes mel- litus in man, there is an increase in the quantity of urine (polyuria) and of urea, and an abnormal thirst and hunger. Acetone also is present in the urine. These symptoms in cases of complete extir- pation of the pancreas are followed by emaciation and muscular weakness, which finally end in death in two to four weeks. If the pancreas is incompletely removed, the glycosuria may be serious, * Loewy and Richter, " Archiv f . Physiologie," 1899, suppl. volume, p. 174. t Minkowski, "Archiv f. exper. Pathologie u. Pharmakologie, " 31, 85, 1893. SECRETION OF THE DUCTLESS GLANDS. 781 or slight and transient, or absent altogether, depending upon the amount of pancreatic tissue left. According to the experiments of von Mering and Minkowski on dogs, a residue of one-fourth to one-fifth of the gland is sufficient to prevent the appearance of sugar in the urine, although a smaller fragment may suffice apparently if its physiological condition is favorable. The portion of pancreas left in the body may suffice to prevent glycosuria, partly or com- pletely, even though its connection with the duodenum is entirely interrupted, thus indicating that the suppression of the pancreatic juice is not responsible for the glycosuria. The same fact is shown more conclusively by the following experiments: Glycosuria after complete removal of the pancreas from its normal connections may be prevented partially or completely by grafting a portion of the pancreas elsewhere in the abdominal cavity or even under the skin. So also the ducts of the gland may be completely occluded by liga- ture or by injection of paraffin without causing a condition of per- manent glycosuria. On the basis of these and similar results it is believed that the pancreas forms an internal secretion which passes into the blood and plays an important, indeed, an essential part in the metabolism of sugar in the body. Moreover, considerable evidence has been accumulated to show that the tissue concerned in this important function is not the pancreatic tissue proper, but that composing the so-called islands of Langerhans. In man these islands are scattered through the pancreas, forming spherical or oval bodies that may reach a diameter of as much as one millimeter. The cells in these bodies are polygonal; their cytoplasm is pale, finely granular, and small in amount. The nuclei possess a thick chromatin network which stains deeply. Each island possesses a rich capillary network that resembles somewhat the glomerulus of the kidney. According to Ssbolew,* ligation of the pancreatic duct is followed by a complete atrophy of the pancreatic cells proper, while those of the islands of Langerhans are not affected. Since under these conditions no glycosuria occurs, while removal of the whole organ including the islands is followed by pancreatic diabetes, the obvious conclusion is that the diabetes is due to the loss of the islands. This conclusion is strengthened by reports from the pathological side. A number of recent observers (Opie, Ssbolew, Herzog, et al.) find that in diabetes mellitus in man the islands are markedly af- fected. They show signs of hyaline degeneration or atrophy or in severe cases may be absent altogether. Several theories have been advanced to explain the action of the internal secretion of the pancreas. It has been suggested that the secretion contains an enzyme which is necessary for the hydrol- *"Virchow's Archiv," 168, 91, 1902. 782 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. ysis or oxidation of the sugar of the body and in the absence of this enzyme the sugar accumulates in the blood and is drained off through the kidney. Cohnheim* states that, while the juices ex- pressed from muscle and from pancreas have little effect upon sugar when taken separately, yet when combined they cause a marked disappearance (glycolysis) of sugar added to the mixture. The inference from this result is that the pancreas furnishes a substance which activates the glycolytic enzyme or enzymes of the muscle and thus makes possible the physiological consumption of sugars in the body. Since the pancreas extracts do not lose this property upon boiling it is evident that the activating substance is not an enzyme, but a body of a more stable character. Other investigators adopt an entirely different view of the relation of the pancreas to carbohydrate metabolism. They believe that the internal secre- tion of the pancreas regulates in some way the output of sugar from the liver (and other sugar-producing organs). In the absence of this secretion the liver gives off its glycogen as sugar too rapidly, the sugar contents of the blood are thereby increased (hypergly- cemia) above normal, and the excess passes out in the urine. Kidney. — Tigerstedt and Bergman t state that a substance may be extracted from the kidneys of rabbits which when injected into the body of a living animal causes a rise of blood-pressure. They get the same effect from the blood of the renal vein. They conclude, therefore, that a substance, for which they suggest the name "rennin," is normally secreted by the kidney into the renal blood, and that this substance causes a vasoconstriction. * Cohnheim, "Zeitschrift f. physiolog. Chemie," 39, 336, 1903; also 1904. f " Skandinavisches Archiv f . Physiologie, " 8, 223, 1898 ; see also Brad- ford, "Proceedings of the Royal Society, " 1892. SECTION VIII. NUTRITION AND HEAT PRODUCTION AND REGULATION. CHAPTER XLVII. GENERAL METHODS-HISTORY OF THE PROTEID FOOD. Under the head of nutrition or general metabolism we include usually all those changes that occur in our foodstuffs from the time that they are absorbed from the alimentary canal until they are eliminated in the excretions. In many of these processes the oxygen absorbed from the lungs takes a most important part, and the changes directly due to this element, the physiological oxidations of the body, can not be separated from the general metabolic phe- nomena of the tissues. As was said in another place, the respiratory history of oxygen ceases after this element has reached the tissues; its subsequent participation in the chemical changes of the organ- ism forms an integral part of the nutritional processes. These latter processes are varied and complex and only partially understood. For the sake of simplicity in presentation it is convenient to con- sider separately each of the so-called foodstuffs, — the proteids, albuminoids, carbohydrates, fats, water, and inorganic salts, — and attempt to trace its nutritive history from the time it is absorbed into the blood until it is eliminated from the body in the form of excretory products. Before undertaking this description it is desir- able to call attention to certain general methods and conceptions that have been developed in connection with this part of physiology. Nitrogen Equilibrium. — Among our main foodstuffs the pro- teids (and albuminoids) are characterized by containing nitrogen. After this material is metabolized in the body the nitrogen is elim- inated in various forms, chiefly in the urine, but to a smaller ex- tent in the feces and sweat. In the feces, moreover, there may be present some undigested proteid which, although taken with the food, has never really entered the body. It is evident that the urine, feces (and sweat) may be collected during a given period and analyzed to determine their contents in nitrogen. The sweat is 783 >■ o 784 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. KB J usuSJli neglected except in observations upon conditions in which musgJar activity has been a prominent feature. As a rule, the amouilt of nitrogen is determined by some modification of the Kjel- daK^ method. In principle this method consists in heating the rngtefrial to be analyzed with strong sulphuric acid. The nitrogen " ereby converted to ammonia, which is distilled off and caught i§^ standardized solution of sulphuric acid. By titration the amount f ammonia can be determined, and from this the amount of nitrogen is estimated. Nitrogen forms a definite percentage of the proteid molecule (about 16 per cent.) ; so that if the weight of nitrogen is multiplied by 6.25 the weight of proteid from which it is derived is obtained. If, on the other hand, the nitrogen is determined in the food eaten during the period of the experiment it is evident that a balance may be struck which will determine whether the body is receiving or losing nitrogen. If the balance is even the body is in nitrogen equilibrium, — that is, it is receiving in the food as much nitrogen (or proteid) as it is metabolizing and eliminating in the excreta. If there is a plus balance in favor of the food it is evident that the body is laying on or storing proteid tissue, while if the balance is minus the body must be losing proteid. During the period of growth, in convalescence, etc., the body does store proteid, and under these conditions the balance is in favor of the food nitrogen. But throughout adult life under normal conditions our diet is so regulated by the appetite that a nitrogen equilibrium is maintained through long periods. Under experimental conditions, involving, for instance, a special diet, it often becomes necessary to make the analyses for nitrogen in order to determine whether or not the individual is losing or gaining proteid or is in equilibrium. It is important also to bear in mind that nitrogen or proteid equilibrium may be established at different levels. If, for instance, a man is in nitrogen equilibrium on a diet containing 10 gms. of nitrogen, what will happen if the proteid in this diet is doubled? Our experience teaches us that the extra 10 gms. of nitrogen or 62.5 gms. of proteid is not stored in the body indefinitely. As a matter of fact, the extra proteid is metabolized in the body and nitrogen equilibrium becomes established at a higher level. Whereas under the first condition 62.5 gms. of proteid were eaten and 62.5 gms. of proteid were lost from the body either in the form of nitrog- enous excreta or in the feces as undigested proteid, under the second condition 125 gms. of proteid are eaten and 125 gms. of pro- teid are lost. The total mass of proteid tissue in the body may remain the same, or if any increase takes place at the beginning of the change in diet it soon ceases. Experimentally it is found that there is a certain low limit of proteid which just suffices to maintain nitrogen equilibrium, and between this level and the capacity of GENERAL METHODS — HISTORY OF PROTBID EOOD. 785 the body to digest and absorb proteid food rutrogOT^raibtti^art'rCVj^, may be maintained upon any given amount of proteid. Carbon Equilibrium and Body Equilibrium. — The term car- bon equilibrium is sometimes used to describe the condition in which the total carbon of the excreta (in the carbon dioxid, urea, etc.) is balanced by the carbon of the food. It is possible that an individual may be in nitrogen equilibrium and yet be losing or gaining in weight, since, although the consumption of proteids may just be covered by the proteids of the food, the consumption of non-proteid material, particularly the fats of the body, may be greater than the supply furnished by or manufactured from the food. An animal may lose or gain in carbon when his nitrogen supply is in equilibrium. In the same way under special circumstances we may speak of a water equilibrium or a salts equilibrium, although these terms are not generally used. An adult under normal conditions lives so as to maintain a general body equilibrium; his ingesta of all kinds are balanced by the corresponding excretions, and the individual maintains a practically constant body-weight. Complete Balance Experiments — Respiration Chamber. — Ac- cording to the statements made in the last paragraph, it is obvious that if the analytical work is properly done, an exact balance may be drawn between the proteids, fats, and carbohydrates eaten as food and the proteids, fats, and carbohydrates destroyed in the body as represented by the nitrogen and carbon contained in the excreta. Complete experiments of this kind were attempted first by Voit * and Pettenkofer, to whose work much of our fundamental knowledge is due. In the experiments of these authors made upon men as well as animals the total nitrogen of the urine and feces was determined and the total quantity of C02 given off from the lungs was estimated. This last determination was made possible by placing the individual in a specially constructed chamber or respi- ration apparatus. Air was drawn through this room by means of a pump. The total quantity of air passing through the room was measured by a gasometer and definite fractions were drawn off from time to time for analysis of its C02. From the figures thus obtained it was possible to estimate the entire C02 given off during the period of observation. Knowing the total nitrogen and C02 eliminated, it is possible to estimate the amount of proteid and fat or carbohydrate destroyed in the body. If the carbon belong- ing to the amount of proteid metabolized is deducted from the total carbon excreta what is left represents either fat or carbohydrate burnt in the body, and, knowing the amount of these materials taken in the diet, it is possible to ascertain whether the correspond- ing amount of carbon has all been excreted. By experiments of * See Hermann's "Handbuch der Physiologie, " vol. vi, 1881. 50 786 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. this kind a nearly perfect balance may be struck between the in- come and the outgo of the body. Absolute accuracy is not sought for, since the materials eaten vary somewhat in composition and some little of the carbon or nitrogen excreted is found in the secre- tions from the skin, the saliva, etc., which are not usually examined. More recent experiments made in this country under the direc- tion of Atwater* have attempted to balance not only the material income and outgo of the body during a given period, but also the income and outgo of energy. For this purpose the individuals ex- perimented upon were placed in a very carefully constructed respi- ration chamber so that their expired air could be analyzed as well as the urine and feces. The chamber, however, was also arranged to act as a calorimeter (see p. 823) by means of which the heat given off by the person could be measured. The heat value of the diet being known, it is possible in this way to ascertain whether or not this theoretical amount of heat is actually given off from the body. Atwater's respiration chamber is described as a respiration calorim- eter; some of the results obtained from its use are referred to later on. The Effect of Non-proteid Food on Nitrogen Equilibrium. — By use of the methods referred to above the general influence of the non-proteid foods (fats, carbohydrates, albuminoids) upon the proteid consumption of the body has been made evident. An ani- mal may be brought into nitrogen equilibrium on proteid food alone, the amount of proteid required being relatively large. If now the proteid food is mixed with any of the non-proteid foodstuffs it is found that the amount of proteid necessary to maintain nitrogen equilibrium may be reduced correspondingly. With reference to the consumption of proteid in the body the non-proteid foods are all proteid-sparers, and herein lies one great peculiarity of their nu- tritional value. On a mixed diet of proteid and non-proteid food the proportion of the latter may be increased and that of the former decreased to a marked extent without breaking down nitrogen equilibrium, — that is, without causing a loss of proteid tissue from the body. This fact is explained by the consideration that the pro- teid of our food fulfills two general functions: Its metabolism or its oxidation furnishes energy, especially heat energy to the body, and, moreover, a portion of it is used to reconstruct the living proto- plasm that breaks down in the functional activity of the tissues. The non-proteid food also furnishes heat energy and work energy, and to a large extent at least can replace this part of the function fulfilled by the proteid. The Nutritive History of the Proteid Food. — The digestive changes undergone by proteid and its subsequent absorption have been described in the section on digestion. It will be remembered * Atwater, Bulletins 45, 63, 69, United States Department of Agriculture. GENERAL METHODS — HISTORY OF PROTEID FOOD. 787 that the products of proteid digestion are absorbed mainly into the blood-vessels of the intestine, and therefore must pass through the liver before reaching the general circulation. It will also be remem- bered that we are as yet ignorant of the precise form in which these products enter the portal blood. This deficiency in our knowledge constitutes a serious obstacle to a satisfactory explanation of the nutritional history of the proteid. Three general views have been advanced concerning the ultimate fate of the absorbed material. In two of these theories it is assumed that the digested material is synthesized into a new proteid, before or after absorption, being converted into what we might call a body proteid characteristic of the animal. Although it is not specifically stated, the assumption seems to be that this body proteid is the serum-albumin of the animal's blood. Accepting this general assumption, one theory, advocated by Pfluger, supposes that before undergoing physiological oxidation all of this absorbed material is built up into the living protoplasm of the various tissues and then undergoes the characteristic metab- olism (catabolism or disassimilation) of that tissue. The second theory, advanced by Voit, assumes that some of the absorbed material is assimilated to form living protoplasm, so far as this is necessary to replace the wastes of the tissue or to provide new material for growth. The portion of the absorbed proteid that subserves this function is designated as tissue proteid. It is obvious that this function can not be replaced by the non-proteid — that is, the non-nitrogenous — foodstuffs. The larger portion of the absorbed material, however, after distribution to the tissues is destroyed, with liberation of heat, under the influence of the activity of the living cells, but without actually becoming transformed into living matter. The cells act toward this material as the yeast cells do toward the sugar that they decompose into alcohol and carbon dioxid. The portion of the proteid that undergoes this fate is designated as the circulating proteid on the hypothesis that it enters the circulating liquids of the body, the blood and lymph, and is carried around in them until destroyed by the tissues. The third general point of view has not been formulated very definitely, but represents perhaps the trend of modern investigation.* According to this theory, the split products of proteid digestion, the monamido- and diamido-bodies — leucin, tyrosin, arginin, etc. — are not wholly built up into a new body proteid. Some of the ma- terial must be so synthesized, either in the intestine or in the tis- sues, to provide material for the regeneration of the wastes of the body, and it will be remembered that, as stated by Abderhalden p. 708), there is some evidence that a portion of the proteid mole- cule during digestion is not broken up into the ultimate split prod- 788 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. ucts, but remains in the more or less complex form indicated by the term polypeptid. This portion may serve as a nucleus for the reconstruction of a body proteid suitable for assimilation into the living structure of the cells. On the other hand, it is known that some of the split products of the digestion of proteid, — the am- monia, the leucin, etc., — when circulated through the liver, give rise to urea. Since the split products of proteid digestion are car- ried at once to the liver, it is possible that this fate overtakes them, and that the nitrogen contained in them is at once converted to urea and prepared for elimination, wThile a portion of the rest of the mole- cules from which the nitrogen is thus removed is retained in the body to be subsequently oxidized and furnish heat energy. This non-nitrogenous residue may first be converted to sugar or fat before its final oxidation. The characteristic feature of this view is the belief that a large part of the nitrogen of the proteid food is promptly converted to urea and is eliminated before becoming a part either of the living proteid or the circulating proteid of the body. This view seems to be opposed to our conceptions of the importance of proteid foods, but it is in harmony with the surprising fact that the digestive enzymes are adapted to split the proteid molecule into what we may call its ultimate products, the relatively simple amido- bodies, and moreover that most of the proteid food taken into our bodies reappears, so far as its nitrogen is concerned, in a few hours as urea in the urine. Folin* has called attention to the fact that the proportions of the different nitrogen compounds in the urine vary with the amount of proteid food. Upon an average diet containing 16 to 17 gms. of nitrogen (100 to 106 gms. of usable proteid) the urea forms 87 to 88 per cent, of the total nitrogen of the urine, while when the proteid intake is reduced to 3 or 4 gms. of nitrogen the urea forms only 61 to 62 per cent, of the total nitrogen of the urine. On the other hand, the creatinin and the purin bodies (uric acid, xanthin, etc.) are not diminished in amount with a decrease in the proteid food. He suggests, therefore, that the latter bodies, creatinin and purin bases and perhaps a part of the other nitrogenous waste prod- ucts, represent the waste of the breaking down of the living tissues, "the catabolism or wear and tear of the living machinery. The urea, on the other hand, represents in large part the nitrogen of that por- tion of the proteid food which, from the present point of view, is hydrolyzed during digestion into the split products and is changed to urea in the liver. The Amount of Proteid Necessary for Normal Nutrition — Luxus Consumption. — As was stated above, nitrogen equilibrium may be maintained on different amounts of proteid food. It is * Folin, "American Journal of Physiology," 13, 45, 66, and 117, 1905. GENERAL METHODS — HISTORY OF PROTEID FOOD. 789 important, from a scientific and from an economic standpoint, to determine the low limit for this equilibrium and to ascertain whether, for the purposes of the best as well as the most economical nutrition, this low limit is as good as or preferable to a higher amount of pro- teid in the diet. Examination of the dietaries of civilized races shows that, on the average, 100 to 120 gms. of proteid are used daily by an adult man. Voit gives 118 gms. of proteid as the average daily consumption. A variable portion of this amount passes into the feces in undigested form, but we may assume that about 105 gms. are absorbed and actually metabolized in the body. Experi- ments show, however, that a man may exist in good health upon a much smaller amount per day, as little as 20 to 40 gms.,* provided the non-proteid portion of the diet is increased. The question is whether the large excess of proteid above what is actually necessary for nitrogen equilibrium is beneficial to the body or is harmful, or lastly is merely a waste, or as the older physiologists called it, a luxus consumption. The facts at our command at present are in- sufficient to give a final answer to this question. On the one side, we have the following facts: Some observers (Munk, Rosenheim), from experiments made upon dogs, state that when a low proteid diet is maintained for some time the animals show a marked dis- turbance in digestion and absorption, which may terminate in death. The fact that mankind universally under the guidance of the self-regulating appetite has adopted a high level of proteid food must also be allowed to have considerable weight. With our im- perfect knowledge of all the conditions it is dangerous to assert that this outcome of the processes of natural selection is without important significance. There is also the fact that in the modern treatment of tuberculosis high feeding with proteids constitutes a factor to which much importance is attributed. The inference seems to be that such a diet increases the power of resistance of the tissues toward invading micro-organisms. On the other side, we have the evidence of numerous investigators who have experimented upon themselves that a proteid diet much smaller than that ordinarily used suffices to maintain normal nutrition. Chittenden, especially, in the careful work already referred to, has shown that men in various walks in life, students, athletes, soldiers, may be well nourished, without loss of strength or impairment of the feeling of well-being, on a diet containing 30 to 50 gms. of proteid instead of 118 gms. These observers believe that the excess of proteid usually employed is undesirable in that it increases the amount of injurious nitrog- enous waste products, that it throws an unnecessary amount of labor upon the excretory organs, and that it increases the pos- * Consult Chittenden, " Physiological Economy in Nutrition," New York, 1905, for discussion and literature. 790 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. sibility of the formation of toxic products in the intestines from putrefactive processes, etc. It may be said, however, that although these experimenters have shown that normal conditions may be maintained for six months to a year upon a low proteid diet, they do not demonstrate satisfactorily that a larger proteid diet is actually attended by evil consequences. The Specific Character of the Proteid Metabolism.— From the fact that the nitrogen is eliminated from the body in various forms — ammonia compounds, urea, creatinin, purin bodies, etc. — we may infer that the specific metabolism varies in the different tissues and under different conditions. The steps or stages of this metabolism are not completely known in any case, but the follow- ing suggestions have been developed by experimental work. Urea arises in part in the liver by a conversion of ammonia salts, — for ex- ample, ammonium carbamate or carbonate (p. 754). The ammonia salts in turn may arise in the tissues by a process of hydrolytic cleavage giving origin to amido-bodies, — leucin, glycocoll, etc.,— which are subsequently oxidized to ammonia, carbon dioxid, and water. The ammonia and carbon dioxid unite to form ammonium carbamate, CO\f)]sjfr > which is then dehydrated, directly or in- 4 directly, by the liver to form urea. Or, as previously described, some of the amido-bodies formed from the proteid during digestion may be carried to the liver, and the amido-group be split off to form urea. The general idea, therefore, is that the nitrogen of the pro- teid is by processes of hydrolysis or by hydrolysis and oxidation converted to ammonia salts and then to urea. Ammonia com- pounds are found in the blood, particularly in that of the portal vein, and these compounds increase in quantity when the liver is removed.* Uric acid and the other purin bodies arise probably from a metabolism of the nuclein material contained in the nucleoproteids of the body, but the processes involved are not known. Presumably they are formed from nuclein by processes of hydrolysis caused by specific enzymes or by the more direct influence of the living tissue. In the laboratory the nucleins may be split by the hydrolytic action of acids with the formation of purin bases. The creatinin of the urine is formed from the creatin found in muscular tissue. Presumably this creatin is derived from a metab- olism of the living muscular tissue. There is evidence to show that increased muscular activity may be associated with an in- creased formation of creatin (p. 759). * See Nencki, Pawlow, and Zaleski, " Archiv f . exp. Pathologie u. Phar- makologie," 37, 26, 1895; and Nencki and Pawlow, "Archives des sciences biologiques," 5, 213. GENERAL METHODS — HISTORY OF PROTEID FOOD. 791 With regard to the non-nitrogenous portion of the proteid mole- cule the evidence to show that it may be converted to glycogen (sugar) has already been given (p. 733), while the views regarding its conversion to fat will be referred to in the following chapter under the head of Origin of the Body Fat. Nutritive Value of Albuminoids. — The albuminoid most fre- quently occurring in food is gelatin. It is derived from collagen of the connective tissues. Collagen of bones or of connective tissue takes up water when boiled and becomes converted into gelatin. We eat gelatin, therefore, in boiled meats, soups, etc,, and, besides, it is frequently employed directly as a food in the form of table gelatin. Collagen has the following percentage composition: C, 50.75 per cent.; H, 6.47; N, 17.86; O, 24.32; S, 0.6. It resembles the proteid molecule closely in percentage composition, and it would seem that the tissues might use it as they do proteid for the for- mation of new protoplasm. Experiments, however, have demon- strated clearly that this is not the case. Animals fed upon albu- minoids together with fats and carbohydrates do not maintain nitrogen equilibrium. The final result of such a diet would be continued loss of weight and malnutrition and death. Gelatin, however, is readily digested, gelatoses and gelatin peptones and event- ually some split products being formed; these are absorbed and oxidized in the body, with the formation of C02, H20, and urea or some related nitrogenous product. Gelatin serves, then, as a source of energy to the body in the same sense as do carbohydrates and fats. When any one of these three substances is used in a diet, the proportion of proteid necessary for the maintenance of nitrogen equilibrium may be reduced greatly. Actual experiments have shown that gelatin is more efficacious than either fats or carbohy- drates in protecting the proteid in the body. The relative value of fats, carbohydrates, and gelatin in protecting proteid from de- struction in the body is illustrated by an experiment reported by Voit: A dog weighing 32 kgms. was fed alternately upon proteid and sugar, proteid and fat, and proteid and gelatin, with the follow- ing result: Nourishment (Gms.) Calculated Destruction of Meat. Gelatin. Fat. Sugar. Flesh in Body (Gms.). 400 — 200 — 450 400 — 250 439 400 200 — — 356 Practically, however, the use of gelatin in diets is restricted by its unpalatableness when employed in large quantities. Whatever may be the physiological cause of this peculiarity, there seems to be no doubt that when used largely in the diet both animals and men soon develop such an aversion to it that it is necessary to dis- continue its use. Munk. has attempted to determine how far the 792 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. proteids of food may be replaced by gelatin. In his experiments a dog was brought into a condition of nitrogenous equilibrium upon a diet of meat, rice, and lard, containing 9.73 gms. of nitrogen. During the period this diet was continued the animal, whose weight was 16.5 kgms., was oxidizing in its body 3.7 gms. of proteid daily for each kilogram of weight. In a second period lasting four days the quantities of rice and lard were the same as before, but the pro- teid in the diet was reduced to 8.2 gms., or 1.3 gms. of nitrogen; the balance of the necessary nitrogen was supplied in the form of gela- tin, so that in round numbers only one-seventh of the required daily amount of nitrogen was given as proteid. The result was that the animal maintained its nitrogen equilibrium for the short period stated. It was found that the experiments could not be continued longer than four days, owing to the growing dislike of the animal for the gelatin food. During the second period the animal was receiving in its food and burning in its body only 0.5 gm. of pro- teid daily for each kilogram of weight, as against 3.7 gms. upon a normal diet. CHAPTER XLVIII. NUTRITIVE HISTORY OF CARBOHYDRATES AND FATS. The Carbohydrate Supply of the Body. — The available carbo- hydrate material of the body consists of the glycogen found in the tissues, especially in the liver (1 to 4 per cent, or more) and mus- cles (0.5 per cent.), and the sugar formed from this glycogen and present constantly in the blood to the amount of 0.1 to 0.15 per cent. In addition it is believed that during starvation glycogen or sugar may be made from the proteid tissues of the body, and possibly also from the body fat, although this latter source is doubtful. The supply of glycogen under normal conditions is main- tained chiefly by the carbohydrate food. As was explained in the section on digestion, the starches, sugars, gums, etc., which con- stitute the carbohydrate foodstuffs, are eventually absorbed into the blood as simple sugars, chiefly dextrose, but probably also some levulose and galactose. These simple sugars constitute the important glycogen formers. There is still some difference of opinion as to whether all proteids are capable of yielding glycogen to the body. Some physiologists believe that after the nitrogen is split off to form urea, the non-nitrogenous portion of the molecule may be con- verted to glycogen in the liver. Others hold that only those pro- teids, such as egg-albumin, which contain a carbohydrate grouping in the molecule are capable of yielding glycogen in the body.* Whether the fats of the food may serve as a source of glycogen is also an open question, but the balance of evidence is probably against such a view. The store of glycogen in the body is about equally divided between the liver and the muscular tissues, but the regulation of the supply of sugar to the blood is usually attributed to the liver. This regulation is adjusted so that the percentage of sugar in the blood is kept astonishingly constant, not only during the conditions of ordinary living, but under such an abnomal con- dition as prolonged starvation. It is assumed that this regulation is effected mainly by an enzyme formed in the liver cells which converts the glycogen to dextrose in proportion as the sugar of the blood is used up by the tissues. The Final Fate of the Carbohydrate of the Body. — Eventually * See Pfluger, in " Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 96, 1, 1903, for literature and discussion. 793 794 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. the carbohydrate of the body is oxidized in the tissues with the formation of carbon dioxid and water. Much uncertainty prevails, however, as to the steps and means by which this oxidation is effected. Reference has already been made to the important fact that the internal secretion of the pancreas (islands of Langerhans) is necessary to this process. According to Cohnheim's experiments, this secretion furnishes an activating substance which enables the enzymes of the muscles and other tissues to attack the sugar. While the matter is still one for investigation, the trend of recent work indicates that the sugar is broken down and oxidized by the suc- cessive action of a number of enzymes, with the formation, there- fore, of a number of intermediate products.* The first step seems to be a conversion to lactic acid CcH1206 = 2C3Hfl03 Sugar. Lactic acid. under the influence of an enzyme (lactolase or zymase). The lactic acid is then split into alcohol and carbon dioxid C3H603 = C02 + C2H5OH Lactic acid. Ethyl-alcohol. by the action of another enzyme (alcoholase or lactacidase), and finally by the action of one or more oxidizing enzymes the alcohol is oxidized to carbon dioxid and water. Whether or not this specific process takes place in normal metabolism must be determined by future work. We are certain at present only of the fact that the final products are carbon dioxid and water, — that is, complete oxidation products, — and that in some way the internal secretion of the pancreas is essential to the process, either in the way indi- cated above or in controlling the activity of the enzyme in the liver which converts the glycogen to sugar. The Temporary Fate of the Carbohydrate in the Tissues. — The sugar absorbed from the alimentary canal may be stored in the body in two forms : First, as explained above, it may be changed to glycogen; or, second, it may be converted into fat and thus be stored in more permanent form. Nutritive experiments, described below, leave no doubt that the fat of the body may be formed from carbohydrate food. It is stated that the fat that has this origin, carbohydrate fat, is of a more solid consistency — that is, has a larger percentage of palmitin and stearin — than the fat coming from other sources. Functions of the Carbohydrate Food. — The general value of the carbohydrate food to the organism may be considered under three heads: (1) It furnishes a source of energy for muscular * See Buchner and Meisenheimer, " Berichte d. deutsch. chem. Gesell- schaft," 38, 620, 1905; and Stoklase, ibid., p. 664. CARBOHYDRATES AND FATS. 795 work. It will be remembered that the glycogen of a muscle dis- appears in proportion to the work done by the muscle, and indeed prolonged muscular work, especially during starvation, may wipe out quickly the entire store of glycogen in the body, in the liver as well as in the muscles. It is usually believed, therefore, that the oxidation of the sugar furnishes energy which by the machinery of the muscles is utilized to do work,— that is, to cause muscular contractions. It seems probable that under normal conditions this material furnishes the main, if not the sole source of energy for muscular work. (2) The oxidation of the sugar furnishes an im- portant part of the constant supply of heat needed by the body. Each gram of sugar on oxidation yields 4 Calories of heat, and, since the carbohydrates form the largest part of our diet and are easily oxidized in the body, they must be regarded as an especially available material for keeping up the supply of animal heat. The largest part of the energy liberated by the oxidation of sugar in the muscles during contraction takes the form of heat, and even dur- ing muscular rest the condition of tone is probably attended by a constant oxidation of this material. (3) The oxidation of the sugar protects the proteid of the body. Attention has already been called to the fact that an animal may be kept in nitrogen equilibrium on a much smaller proteid diet provided carbohydrates (or fats) are also eaten. One may say, in fact, that as the carbohydrate food is increased the proteid food may be diminished, down to a certain irreducible minimum. From the chemical composition of carbo- hydrates it is evident that they alone can not serve to build up pro- toplasm. Some proteid food is absolutely necessary for the repair of tissue or the production of new tissue during growth. An ani- mal fed on carbohydrate food alone, no matter how abundant the supply, would eventually starve to death. Within the limits speci- fied, however, the carbohydrates are proteid-sparers ; the energy provided by their oxidation keeps up the supply of heat and enables the muscles and probably the other tissues to function normally, and to this extent protects the living proteid from consumption and enables us to reduce the proteid material in our diet. The im- portance of the carbohydrates in nutrition is illustrated in a strik- ing way by the facts in diabetes. Diabetes occurs in man (diabetes mellitus) as a pathological condition which in some cases at least is referable to a lesion of the pancreas. As already described, it may also be produced in dogs and other animals by removal of the pancreas (pancreatic diabetes). Under both conditions when com- plete the carbohydrates eaten are not oxidized in the body, but are eliminated as sugar in the urine. Corresponding to the loss of this source of energy an increased amount of proteid is necessary in the diet, and adding carbohydrate to the food in such cases does not 796 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. alter the nitrogen elimination in the urine. The diabetic condition may be produced in other ways the most interesting of which is by the administration of phloridzin, — a vegetable glucosid obtained from the roots of certain trees (apple, pear). The cause of phlo- ridzin diabetes is not entirely known, but the general view is that the glucosid acts upon the kidney and renders it more permeable, so that the sugar of the blood escapes into the urine, or, on the other hand, it may affect the condition of the sugar in the blood so as to make it pass through the kidney more easily. The diabetes in this case has obviously a different cause from that of pancreatic diabetes, but by repeated use of the drug the loss of carbohydrate to the body may be made complete. If the animal is fed only on proteid, proteid and fat, or is starved so that he is living on the proteid and fat of his own body the drug still causes the appear- ance of large quantities of sugar in the urine. Lusk* finds under such conditions that the amount of dextrose in the urine bears a definite ratio to the amount of nitrogen, — D : N :: 3.65 : 1. He believes that the establishment of this ratio is an indication of a complete inability of the tissues to use sugar. A similar ratio has been obtained in the urine of individuals suffering from a severe and eventually fatal form of diabetes mellitus. Assuming that the sugar is these cases comes entirely from the proteid of the food or of the tissues, this ratio would indicate that 58.4 per cent, of the proteid molecule may be converted to sugar in the body. Nutritive Value of Fats. — The fats of food are absorbed into the lacteals chiefly as neutral fats, — the so-called chyle fat. They eventually reach the blood in this condition, and are afterward in some way oxidized by the tissues. The final products of their oxidation are the same as when burnt outside the body — namely, C02 and H20 — and a corresponding amount of energy must be liberated. Speaking generally, then, the essential nutritive value of the fats is that they furnish energy to the body, and, from a chemical standpoint, they must contain more available energy, weight for weight, than the proteids or the carbohydrates. In a well-nourished animal a large amount of fat is found normally in adipose tissues, particularly in the so-called "panniculusadiposus" beneath the skin, and in the folds of the peritoneum, etc. Physi- ologically, this body fat is to be regarded as a reserve supply of nourishment. When food is eaten and absorbed in excess of the actual metabolic processes of the body, the excess is stored in the adipose tissue as fat, to be drawn upon in case of need — as, for instance, during partial or complete starvation. A starving animal, after its small supply of glycogen is exhausted, lives entirely upon * See Lusk and coworkers, "American Journal of Physiology," vols, i, iii, and x; also " Deutsches Archiv f. klin. Med.," 81, 472, 1904. CARBOHYDRATES AND FATS. 797 body proteids and fats; the larger the supply of fat, the more effectively will the proteid tissues be protected from destruction. In accordance with this fact, it has been shown that when subjected to complete starvation a fat animal survives longer than a lean one. Our supply of fat is called upon not only during complete abstention from food, but also whenever the diet is insufficient to cover the oxidations of the body, as in deficient food, sickness, etc. The Fate of the Fat in the Tissues. — The fat absorbed as food may temporarily subserve several different purposes: (1) It may be oxidized with the formation of heat energy. (2) It may be stored in the tissues as part of the body fat. (3) It may be synthesized with other substances to form some more complex constituent of the body, such as lecithin. (4) According to some authors, it may serve under certain conditions as a source of sugar. This latter suggestion is not supported by convincing experiments. The final fate of the fat in the body is, however, to be oxidized to water and carbon dioxid. The nature of the processes involved is not understood. It is generally believed, however, that the first step is the splitting of the fat into fatty acid and glycerin under the influence of the lipase found in so many of the tissues of the body. The fat that lies in the storage tissues — skin, peri- toneum, etc. — probably does not undergo oxidation in these places. In times of need it is absorbed and distributed to the more active tissues, and in this initial process of solution it is probable that a regulative influence is exerted by the lipase as suggested by Loeven- hart (see p. 660). That is, by its reversible action this enzyme may control the output of fat to the blood, as the supply of sugar in the blood is kept constant by the diastatic enzyme of the liver. After the action of the lipase we can only say that oxidation takes place, but through how many stages is not known. Origin of the Body Fat. — The views upon the origin of body fat have undergone a number of changes in the last fifty or sixty years, illustrating in an interesting way how development of our experimental methods leads often at first to half-truths which are corrected later by more extensive work. Dumas and others (1840) held to the natural view that the fat of the body originates directly from the fat of the food. Liebig, applying his more exact methods, demonstrated that in some cases at least this source is insufficient to account for all the fat. The fat yielded by the milk of a cow for instance, may be greater in quantity than the fat contained in the food. He also pointed out that the fat of each species of animal is more or less peculiar, the fat of the sheep having a higher melting point than pork fat, and both differing in composition from the fat taken as food. "In hay or the other fodder of oxen no beef suet exists, and no hog's lard can be found in the potato refuse 798 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. given to swine." He was led to attribute the source of body fat chiefly to the carbohydrate food, and this belief agreed well with the experience of agriculturists as to the use of such foods in fatten- ing animals for market. This view, in turn, was displaced by the theory of Voit, supported by elaborate feeding experiments. Voit believed that the fat of the body is formed mainly or entirely from the proteid of the food, the carbohydrate and the fat of the diet being useful only to protect a part of this proteid from oxidation. Voit's experiments have been shown by Pfliiger to have been based upon erroneous analyses of the meat used in his experiments. Voit assumed that in this meat the ratio -$- is equal to 1.34 to 1.37, while Pfliiger showed that it is lower,* 1.33. The modern point of view is that the fat of the body originates partly from the fat of the food, particularly in carnivora, and partly from the carbohydrate of the food, especially in herbivora, in whose diet this foodstuff forms such a large part. Whether under any circumstances the pro- teid food may also serve as a source of body fat is still an open question, decisive experiments being lacking. Origin of Body Fat from Food Fat. — The first proofs that the food fats may be deposited as such in the fat tissues of the body were obtained by feeding foreign fats to dogs and demon- strating that these fats can afterward be recognized in the tissues of the animals, f Linseed oil, rape-seed oil, and mutton-fat were used in these experiments. Secondly, it has been made probable by feeding experiments that the normal fat of the food undergoes a similar fate. Thus, Hofmann used a dog weighing 26 kgms. and allowed it to starve until its weight was reduced to 16 kgms. It was then fed for five days on a little meat and large quantities of fat. At the end of that time it was killed and analyzed. The body contained 1353 gms. of fat, of which only 131 gms. could have come from the proteid used, assuming that this material can serve as a fat former. Much of the fat found, therefore, was probably derived from the fat of the food. Origin of Body Fat from Carbohydrates. — That the body fat may have this origin has been made probable or certain by feeding experiments. Thus, Rubner fed a dog (5.89 kgms.) for two days on a diet of sugar, starch, and fat whose total carbon content was equal to 176.6 gms. During this period the animal excreted 87.1 gms. of carbon. There were retained in the body, therefore, 89.5 gms. carbon. The fat fed, 4.7 gms., contained (4.7 X 0.77) 3.6 gms. C. The total nitrogen excreted during this * Pfliiger, "Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 51, 229, 1892, and 77, 521, 1899. t Lebedeff, " Centralblatt f. die med. Wiss.," 8, 1881, and Munk, " Vir- chow's Archiv, " 95, 407, 1884. CARBOHYDRATES AND FATS. 799 period was 2.55 gms., which indicated a metabolism, therefore, of 16 gms. (2.55 X 6.25) of body proteid. Making the improbable assumption that all of the carbon of this proteid was retained in the -body, this would account for 8.32 gms. C (16 X 0.52); so that 3.6 + 8.32 or 12 gms. C might have originated from sources other than the carbohydrate of the food, leaving, therefore, 89.5 — 12 or 77.5 gms. of C which could have arisen only from the carbohy- drate. This quantity of carbon could have been retained only as glycogen or fat. Allowing for the greatest possible storage of glycogen, 78 gms. or 34.6 gms. C, there would still remain 42.9 gms. of C which could have been retained only as fat. Numerous other fattening experiments of different kinds have been made in which it has been shown that the fat laid on by the animal could not be accounted for by the fat of the food or by assuming with Voit that it originated from the proteid. The combined testimony of these experiments have satisfied physiologists that the fat tissues can produce fat from sugar. The chemistry of the change is not under- stood and can not be imitated in the laboratory. The Source of Body Fat in Ordinary Diets. — For the pur- poses of demonstration the experiments made to prove the origin of body fat from carbohydrate or the fat of food have made use of abnormal diets and conditions. It would be a matter of practical interest to ascertain whether upon normal diets the fat of the body originates most easily from the fat or from the carbohydrate of the food. While the question is one to which a positive answer can not be given, it seems to be probable that the result varies with conditions and the nature of the animal. Experience seems to show that carnivorous animals can be fattened more easily on a fat diet, herbivora on a carbohydrate diet. In animals like our- selves there is reason to believe that the carbohydrates are more easily and more quickly destroyed in the body than the fats, and that therefore the latter may be more readily deposited in the tis- sues, although an excess of carbohydrate beyond the actual needs of the body may also be preserved in the form of fat or glycogen.* The Cause of the Deposit of Body Fat — Obesity. — Our experience shows that individuals differ greatly in the ease with which they form fat. Some upon relatively small diets form much fat, while others remain thin in spite of the ingestion of large amounts of food. Voit has indicated the general reason for this difference, — namely, that it depends upon the capacity of the body to destroy food material. When food is supplied and absorbed in excess of this capacity the excess is stored mainly as fat, and to a small extent as glycogen or as new proteid. A diet, there- fore, which will give such an excess to one individual may in the * Consult Rosenfeld, " Ergebnisse der Physiologie, " vol. i, parti, 1902. Complete literature. 800 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. body of another of the same weight be all consumed. The oxidizing capacity of the body differs in different individuals and some will lay on fat more readily than others, because for them an excess of material is provided by a relatively small diet. Fundamental differences of this character in the properties of the protoplasm are frequently transmitted by heredity through many generations. Those individuals who show little tendency to lay on fat may be made to do so by largely increasing the amount of food, or more certainly by altering the mode of life. A sedentary life, absence of worry, etc., may lead to a tendency of this kind, while a very active muscular life has the opposite effect. Men who lead a very muscular life — farmers, fishermen, etc. — are rarely disposed to ac- cumulate fat to a noticeable degree. So also the use of alcoholic beverages may indirectly favor accumulation of fat, presumably by depressing the oxidizing capacity of the tissues. The tendency to form fat may exhibit itself in some cases to such an extent as to con- stitute an almost pathological condition. Extreme obesity may be counteracted by altering the mode of life, especially by . taking much muscular exercise, or more directly by dieting. The diet for such purposes should be reduced in amount, and should be as free as possible from fats and carbohydrates, consisting of such material as eggs, fish, lean meat, salads, fruits, etc. The well-known Banting diet, devised by a London physician (1864) for the cure of obesity, makes use of this latter principle.* Summary of the General Functions of Fat. — The general functions fulfilled by the fats may be summarized briefly under the following heads: (1) It provides a store of reserve food which is used by the body in case of deficiency of food or complete starva- tion. The fattening of hibernating animals before their winter sleep and the humps of the camel give conspicuous examples of this peculiarity. (2) By its oxidation in the body it furnishes a part of the heat energy necessary to maintain the body temperature. On account of its high combustion equivalent (1 gm. of fat yields 9.3 Calories) fat is very effective in this respect. Inhabitants of cold regions choose a diet rich in fat. (3) It is a proteid-saver. Like the carbohydrate food, its oxidation protects the proteid from consumption. In starvation, therefore, the amount of proteid destroyed daily is smaller as long as any fat remains, and, under ordinary conditions of life the larger the amount of fat in the diet, the less the amount of proteid necessary for maintaining the body in nitrogen equilibrium. Experiments show that in this respect the fat is not so effective as an equivalent amount of carbohydrate food. The difference is referable probably to the greater difficulty of oxidation of the fatty material. * For practical directions concerning the treatment of obesity by dieting see Gautier, " L'alimentation et les regimes." Paris, 1904. CHAPTER XLIX. NUTRITIVE VALUE OF THE INORGANIC SALTS AND THE ACCESSORY ARTICLES OF DIET. The Inorganic Salts. — The body contains in its tissues and liquids a considerable amount of inorganic material. When any organ is incinerated this material remains as the ash. If we omit the bones, which are rich in mineral material, the average amount of ash in the body amounts to about 0.1 per cent, of its weight. It consists of chlorids, phosphates, sulphates, carbonates, fluorids, or silicates of potassium, sodium, calcium, magnesium, and iron; iodin occurs also, especially in the thyroid tissues. In the liquids of the body the main salts are sodium chlorid, sodium carbonate, sodium phosphate, potassium and calcium chlorid or phosphate. In considering the organic foodstuffs weight was laid upon their value as sources of energy, as well as upon their function in con- structing tissue. The salts, on the contrary, have no importance from the former standpoint. Whatever chemical changes they undergo are not attended by any liberation of heat energy, — none at least, of sufficient importance to be considered. They have, however, most important functions. They maintain a normal composition and osmotic pressure in the liquids and tissues of the body, and by virtue of their osmotic pressure they play an im- portant part in controlling the flow of water to and from the tissues. Moreover, these salts constitute an essential part of the composition of living matter. In some way they are bound up in the structure of the living molecule and are necessary to its normal reactions or irritability. Even the proteids of the body liquids contain definite amounts of ash, and if this ash is removed their properties are seriously altered, as is shown by the fact that ash-free native proteids lose their property of coagulation by heat. The globulins are precipitated from their solutions when the salts are removed. The special importance of the calcium salts in the coagulation of blood, and the curdling of milk has been referred to, as also the peculiar part played by the calcium, potassium, and sodium salts in the rhythmical contractions of heart muscle and the irritability of muscular and nervous tissues. The special importance of the iron salts for the production of hemoglobin is also evident without comment. The nutritive importance of the salts in the diet has been demonstrated by direct experiment. 51 801 802 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. Fatal Effects of Ash-free or Ash-poor Diets. — Dogs have been fed (Forster) upon a diet composed of ash-free fats and carbo- hydrates, and meats which had been extracted with water until the salts had been much reduced. The animals were in a moribund condition at the end of 26 to 36 days. It is probable that they would have lived longer if deprived of food entirely, with the excep- tion of water, since the metabolism of the abundant diet provided aided in increasing the loss of salts from the body. Lunin has described experiments which indicate that some at least of our salts must be provided for us in organic combinations such as are found in plant and animal foods. In his experiments he found that mice lived well on a diet of dried cows' milk. If fed, however, on a diet containing the organic but ash-free constituents of milk, — namely, sugar, fat, and casein, — together with the extracted salts of cows' milk, they died in 20 to 30 days. The Special Importance of Sodium Chlorid, Calcium, and Iron Salts. — Sodium chlorid occupies a peculiar position among the inorganic constituents of our diet, in that it is the only one which we deliberately add to our food. The other inorganic material is taken unconsciously in our diet, but although sodium chlorid exists also in our food in relatively large quantities we purposely add more. It is estimated that the average man in- gests from 20 to 30 gms. a day. This peculiarity is exhibited also by many animals. The farmer provides salt for his stock and wild animals visit the salt-licks constantly. Bunge has called attention to the fact that among men and animals this desire for salt is limited, for the most part at least, to those that use vege- table food. From the accounts of travelers he shows that when a purely animal diet is used there is no desire for salt ; but on a vege- table diet there is a craving for it which may become very intense and unpleasant when circumstances prevent its being obtained. He offers an ingenious explanation for this relation. Most vege- tables contain a large amount of potassium salts, and in the blood these salts react with the sodium chlorid. Thus, if potassium sulphate were added to the blood it would react with sodium chlorid, giving some potassium chlorid and some sodium sulphate. Both of these salts will be removed by the kidneys, since, except in minute amounts, they are, so to speak, foreign to the blood. This latter liquid will thereby lose some of its supply of sodium salt, whence the craving for more in the food.* Whether or not this explanation is correct, the fact which it seeks to account for seems to be well established. It can not be doubted, however, that under ordinary conditions we use salt in quantities much larger than * For an interesting discussion, see Bunge, " Physiologie des Menschen, " vol. ii, p. 103, 1901. INORGANIC SALTS, STIMULANTS, AND CONDIMENTS. 803 is necessary to maintain the sodium chlorid content of the blood. It is employed as a condiment for its pleasant flavor, and it is possible that its use is often carried to excess. This is a matter of practical dietetics concerning which at present we have no satisfac- tory experimental data to base a judgment upon. The calcium salts of the body play a most important role in connection with the irritability of muscle and nerve (p. 501). They are also of obvious importance in furnishing material for the growth of the skeleton. Their importance in this regard has been demonstrated by feeding experiments. Young dogs when given a diet poor in calcium salts fall into a condition resembling rickets in children, owing to a deficient growth of the bones. Pig- eons also, when fed upon a similar diet, exhibit an atrophy and fragility of the bones due doubtless to the lack of calcium salts. As in the case of the other food materials, there must be a definite calcium metabolism in the body. It is probable, indeed certain, that most of the calcium salts ingested simply pass through the body without entering into its structure. They are eliminated unchanged or unused in the feces or urine. A small portion, how- ever, must be absorbed and used and a corresponding amount must be eliminated as a true waste product of tissue metabolism. Voit, by experiments upon isolated loops of the intestine, has shown that some calcium is constantly eliminated from the inner surface of the intestine. The amount is small. He estimates it at less than 0.002 gm. CaO daily. There is some evidence that the amount of calcium in the tissues increases with age. This is certainly true of the bones, which become exceedingly brittle in advanced life. The iron salts that are constantly necessary for the production of new hemoglobin are provided in our food, in which they exist in organic combination. The value of the food in this respect varies greatly, as may be seen from the following table selected from Bunge's analysis: 100 gins, of dry substance contains iron in milligrams, as follows : White of egg trace Apples 13 Rice 1 to 2 Cabbage (green leaves) ... 17 Wheat flour (bolted) . . 1.6 Beef 17 Cows' milk 2.3 Asparagus 20 Potatoes 6.4 Yolk of egg 10 to 24 Peas 6.2 to 6.6 Spinach 33 to 39 Carrots 8.6 In conditions of malnutrition, particularly in the simple anemias, it becomes necessary to select a diet with reference to its contents in iron or to add iron deliberately to the diet. Therapeutically iron may be given in the form of simple salts with organic or mineral acids or in more complex organic combination. There has been much controversy as to whether the body is capable of taking 804 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. the iron in inorganic form and synthesizing it into a molecule so complex as that of hemoglobin. Experience, however, seem to show that this is possible, although under normal conditions at least our iron is used in organic form. Bunge first isolated such a compound, a nucleo-albumin containing iron, which he prepared from the egg yolk and called hematogen. This compound must serve as the source of the hemoglobin in the developing chick. When the diet is directed especially toward increasing the iron food it would seem to be wiser to choose these compounds, or, better still, the iron-rich foods, rather than medicinal preparations of the inorganic salts. The daily excretion of iron from the body takes place in the feces rather than in the urine. The experiments of Voit upon isolated loops of the intestine, referred to above, show that iron is eliminated from the walls of the intestine. The whole history of the metabolism of iron in the body is surrounded by much uncertainty. After absorption its synthesis to hemoglobin takes place, as to its final stages, in the red marrow, but it is possible that other organs may take part in the formation of intermediate products. As regards its elimination, we know that the breaking down of the hemoglobin, formation of bile pigments, takes place probably in the liver, but the final excretion of the iron is made in some form through the walls of the intestine. Accessory Articles of Diet. — Under this general term we may include all those bodies classed as condiments, flavors, and stimu- lants which we habitually take in our diet in order to enhance the attractiveness of the food. These substances may or may not have some heat value to the body, — that is, they may undergo oxidation with the liberation of heat energy; but, in general, their value in nutrition lies in other properties. The Flavors and Condiments. — Perhaps the most important influence exerted by these bodies is that by making the food appe- tizing they increase the secretion of gastric juice. The origin of the so-called psychical secretion has been described (p. 689), and there can be little doubt that the palatableness of food influences greatly the facility with which its gastric digestion is accomplished. It is said, in fact, that dogs will refuse to eat food that has been deprived entirely of its sapidity and flavor, preferring rather to starve. Some of these substances (pepper), as also the stimulants (alcohol), may have an additional value in that they increase the rapidity of absorption from the stomach. Gautier divides the condiments into the following classes: (1) Aromatics, comprising vanilla, anise, cinnamon, nutmeg, and other similar essential oils. (2) Peppers. (3) The alliaceous condiments, — garlic, mustard, etc. (4) The acid condiments, — vinegar, citron, pickles, etc. (5) The salty condiments, such as table salt. (6) The sugar condiments. INORGANIC SALTS, STIMULANTS, AND CONDIMENTS. 805 The Stimulants. — Under this head we include alcohol, tea, coffee, chocolate, or cocoa, and meat extracts (beef tea, etc.). Regarding the last mentioned substance, its physiological value has been made clear by the work of Pawlow (p. 689.) Meat extracts of various kinds contain secretogogues which stimulate the gastric glands to secretion. In themselves they may contain very little actual foodstuff. Liebig's extract contains some proteid, gelatin, and gly- cogen, which form an actual nourishment, but its specific value as a gastric stimulant depends upon other constituents, possibly the nitrogenous extractives, — creatin, xanthin, carnin, etc. Coffee and tea owe their well-known stimulating action to the presence of an alkaloid, caffein or trimethyl-xanthin. It may be considered as xanthin in which three of the hydrogen atoms have been re- placed by methyl (CH3) groups, as is indicated in the following structural formulas: HN-CO CH3N— CO CO C— NH CO C— N- „„ /Alcohol-free days. .212.58 gms. carbon. ^jerre \ Alcohol days 220.84 " 4- 8.26 " ni .. /Alcohol-free days. .214.83 gms. carbon. ulopatt \ Alcohol days 22CL87 " " * + 6.04 " These results indicate that the alcohol is used by the body in place of the other carbon-containing foodstuffs. Geppert and Zuntz have also found that on alcohol days there is no material increase in the carbon dioxid eliminated or the oxygen absorbed. Theoretically if the alcohol takes the place of the other material the amount of carbon dioxid excreted should be diminished. One gram of alcohol when oxidized furnishes as much heat as 1.7 gms. of sugar or 0.75 gm. of fat. But 1 gm. of alcohol when burnt yields only 1.91 gms. of C02, while 1.7 gms. of sugar yield 2.77 gms. C02, and 0.75 gm. of fat, 2.13 gms. of C02. * See Atwater and Benedict, Bulletin 69, United States Department of Agriculture, 1899. 808 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. If fat were replaced by the alcohol the amount of C02 should be reduced about 10 per cent., while if the sugar were replaced the reduction should amount to 31 per cent. That such a reduction is not actually observed is explained by the fact that the alcohol leads to more muscular activity and a greater loss of heat from the congested skin, thus indirectly augmenting the oxidations of the body. To determine whether the combustion of the alcohol protects the proteid material from metabolism to the same extent as is done by carbohydrates and fats experiments have been made in which the individual was brought into nitrogen equilibrium on a mixed diet. Then for a given period a portion of the carbohydrate was omitted and alcohol in isodynamic amounts was substituted. The result was an increase in the nitrogen excretion, showing that the alcohol did not protect fully the proteid tissue. In a third period the first diet was resumed, and after nitrogen equilibrium had again been established the same proportion of carbohydrate was omitted from the diet, but this time alcohol was not substituted. If the diet was poor in proteid it was found that less proteid was lost from the body when the alcohol was omitted than when it was used. Hence alcohol not only did not take the place of the carbohydrate in protecting the proteid but actually caused an increased proteid consumption.* Other observers (Neumann, Rosemannf) have found that, although the effect described may occur in the first few days, yet if the alcohol diet is maintained the injurious effect exercised by it disappears, the body ceases to lose its proteid tissue, and may even lay on proteid. These results, taken with those given above, indicate, therefore, that the alcohol may actually take the place physiologically of fat or carbohydrates as a source of energy and as a protector of proteid metabolism. { Under these circumstances, therefore, it acts as a true foodstuff. It is perhaps scarcely necessary to emphasize the fact that this scientific con- clusion does not mean that alcohol can be regarded as a prac- tical food. Its expensiveness, its dangers when the dose is too large, etc., prevent us from regarding it in this light. As Rosemann says, however, it is possible that on account of its ready absorption and palatableness it may form a useful substitute for the solid, non-nitrogenous foodstuffs in sickness. This suggestion seems to be supported by many reports of cases in which alcohol has served as the sole or main nutriment during the critical periods of fevers and in other conditions, but it needs to be tested more carefully by direct experiments before it can be accepted generally for prac- tical purposes. * See Miura, " Zeitschrift fur klin. Medicin, " 20, 1892. t See Rosemann, " Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 86, 307, 1901, and 100, 348, 1903, for discussion and literature. % See also At water and Benedict, " Memoirs of National Academy of Sciences," 1902 ; and Atwater, "The Nutritive Value of Alcohol, " in " Physi- ological Aspects of the Liquor Problem," vol. ii, 1903. CHAPTER L. EFFECT OF MUSCULAR WORK AND TEMPERATURE ON BODY METABOLISM— HEAT ENERGY OF FOODS— DIETETICS. The Effect of Muscular Work. — It is a matter of common knowledge that muscular exercise increases the food consumed, and scientific experiments have shown that it greatly augments the consumption of material in the body. Physiologists have attempted to determine which of our energy-yielding foodstuffs is directly affected by muscular activity. A brief statement of the development of our knowledge upon this point will make clear our present theories. According to Liebig, our foods fulfill two general purposes in the body : they are burnt to supply heat, respira- tory foods — fats, and carbohydrates, or they are used to construct tissue, plastic foods — proteids. It seemed to follow, from this generalization, that muscular tissue in activity should use proteid material, and it was believed at that time that the metabolism of proteid furnishes the source of muscular energy. That it is not the sole source was demonstrated by the interesting experiments of Fick and Wislicenus. These physiologists ascended the Faul- horn to a height of 1956 meters. Knowing the weight of his body, each could estimate how much work was done in ascending such a height. Fick's weight, for example, was 66 kilograms; therefore in climbing the mountain he performed 66X1956=129,096 kilo- grammeters of work. In addition, the work of the heart and the respiratory muscles, which could not be determined accurately, was estimated at 30,000 kilogrammeters. There was, moreover, a certain amount of muscular work performed in the move- ments of the arms and in walking upon level ground that was omitted entirely from their calculations. For seventeen hours before the ascent, during the climb of eight hours, and for six hours afterward their food was entirely non-nitrogenous, so that the urea eliminated came entirely from the proteid of the body. Nevertheless, when the urine was collected and the urea estimated it was found that the energy contained in the proteid destroyed, reckoned as heat energy, was entirely insufficient to account for the work done. Although later estimates would modify somewhat the actual figures of their calculation, the margin was so great that 809 810 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. the experiment has been accepted as showing conclusively that the total energy of muscular work does not come necessarily from the oxidation of proteid. Later experiments made by Voit upon a dog working in a tread-wheel and upon a man performing work while in the respiratory chamber gave the surprising result that not only may the energy of muscular work be far greater than the potential energy of the proteid simultaneously oxidized, but that the performance of muscular work within certain limits does not affect at all the amount of proteid metabolized in the body, since the output of urea is the same on working days as during days of rest. Careful experiments by an English physiologist, Parkes, made upon soldiers while resting and after performing long marches showed also that there is no distinct increase in the secretion of urea after muscular exercise. It followed from these latter experiments that Liebig's theory as to the source of the energy of muscular work is incorrect, and that the increase in the oxidations in the body that undoubtedly occurs during muscular activity must affect only the non-proteid material, — that is, the fats and carbohydrates. Subsequently the question was reopened by experiments made under Pfliiger by Argutinsky.* In these experiments the total nitrogen excreted was determined with especial care in the sweat as well as in the urine and the feces. The muscular work done consisted in long walks and mountain climbs. Argutinsky found that work caused a marked increase in the elimination of nitrogen, the increase extending over a period of three days, and he estimated that the additional proteid metabolized in consequence of the work was sufficient to account for most of the energy expended in per- forming the. walks and climbs. A number of objections have been made to Argutinsky's work. It has been asserted that during his experiment he kept himself upon a diet deficient in non-proteid material, and that if the supply of this material had been sufficient there would not have been any increase in proteid metabolism. These experiments were repeated in various forms by many ob- servers (Zuntz, Speck, et al.), and the general result has been the abandonment of both the former views, — the Liebig theory that the energy comes only from the consumption of proteid, and the Voit theory, that it comes only from the oxidation of non-pro- teid material. It has been found that in muscular work carried to the ordinary extent proteid material, in excess of that destroyed in conditions of rest, may or may not be used according to the amount of fats and carbohydrates in the diet. If these latter elements are in sufficient quantity they furnish the energy required, and proteid metabolism is not increased by work. If, however, the non-pro- * Argutinsky, u Pfluger's Archiv f . die gesammte Physiologie, " 46, 552, 1890. EFFECT OF MUSCULAR WORK AND TEMPERATURE. 811 teids are not sufficient in quantity some of the energy is obtained at the expense of the proteid of the body, and there is an in- crease in the nitrogen excretion. We may believe, in fact, that the energy necessary for muscular work may be obtained from any of the heat-yielding foodstuff, — carbohydrates, fat, or proteids. It seems probable that the sugar (glycogen) of the muscle is, so to speak, the easiest source; but, when the carbohydrates are deficient or absent altogether in the diet, muscular exercise is accompanied by an increase in the consumption of proteids or fats or both. The Voit theory is correct to the extent that on an abundant non-proteid diet much muscular work may be done without any increase in the consumption of proteid tissue. The muscle is a proteid machine for the accomplishment of work, and the fuel supplied may be pro- teid or non-proteid, but in the accomplishment of moderate work there is apparently no greater wear and tear of the machinery, no greater tissue waste, than under resting conditions. If, however, the muscular work is excessive, the tissue waste may be increased. Argutinsky found an increased nitrogen elimination lasting two or three days after the cessation of the work. It is probable that this result indicates a greater waste of the proteid apparatus itself, and this idea is borne out by the fact that under similar conditions other observers have detected an increase in the creatinin excre- tion, the nitrogenous waste that is peculiar to the muscle. The effect of muscular work on the carbon excretion, carbon dioxid, is, of course, marked and invariable. Some extra material must be oxidized to furnish the energy, and since this material is usually or exclusively sugar, or sugar and fat, or the non-nitrogenous portion of the proteid of the diet, the effect, so far as the excretions are con- cerned, will be most manifest in the amount of carbon dioxid given off. Pettenkofer and Voit found that the carbon dioxid eliminated by a man during a day of work was nearly double that excreted during a day of rest. Along with this rise in the carbon dioxid excretion there is a corresponding increase in the absorption of oxygen. Metabolism during Sleep. — It has been shown that during sleep there is no marked diminution of the nitrogen excreted, and therefore no distinct decrease in the proteid metabolism; on the contrary, the C02 eliminated and the oxygen absorbed are unques- tionably diminished. This latter fact finds its simplest explana- tion in the supposition that the muscles are less active during sleep. The muscles do less work in the way of contractions, and, in addition, probably suffer a diminution in tonicity which also affects their total metabolism. Effect of Variations in Temperature. — In warm-blooded animals variations of outside temperature within ordinary limits •'h BR ARY r , ,. 812 .A\NyrfRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. u nu I Effect the body temperature. An account of the means by which this regulation is effected will be found in the chapter upon animal heat. So long as the temperature of the body remains con- stant, it has been found that a fall of outside temperature may increase the oxidation of non-proteid material in the body, the in- crease being in a general way proportional to the fall in tempera- ture. That the increased oxidation affects the non-proteid con- stituents is shown by the fact that the urea remains unchanged in quantity, other conditions being the same, while the oxygen con- sumption and the C02 elimination are increased. This effect of temperature upon the body metabolism is due mainly to a reflex stimulation of the motor nerves to the muscles. The temperature nerves of the skin are affected by a fall in outside temperature, and bring about reflexly an increased innervation of the muscles of the body. Indeed, it is stated* that unless the lowering of the temperature is sufficient to cause shivering or mus- cular tension no increase in the C02 excretion results. This fact suffices to explain, therefore, the physiological value of shivering and muscular restlessness when the outside temperature is low. The fact that variations in outside temperature affect only the consumption of non-proteid material falls in, therefore, with the conception of the nature of the metabolism of muscle in activity, given above. When the means of regulating the body temperature break down from too long an exposure to excessively low or ex- cessively high temperatures, the total body metabolism, proteid as well as non-proteid, increases with a rise in body temperature and decreases with a fall in temperature. In fevers arising from pathological causes it has been shown that there is an in- creased production of urea as well as of C02. Effect of Starvation. — A starving animal must live upon the material present in its body. This material consists of the fat stored up, the circulating and tissue proteid, and the glycogen. The latter, which is present in comparatively small quantities, is quickly used, disappearing more or less rapidly according to the extent of muscular movements made. Thereafter the animal lives on its own proteid and fat, and if the starvation is continued to a fatal termination the body becomes correspondingly emaciated. Examination of the several tissues in animals starved to death has brought out some interesting facts. Voit took two cats of nearly equal weight, fed them equally for ten days, and then killed one to serve as a standard for comparison and starved the other for thirteen days; the latter animal lost 1017 gms. in weight, and the loss was divided as follows among the different organs: * Johannson, " Skandinavisches Archiv f. Physiologie," 7, 123, 1897. EFFECT OF MUSCULAR WORK AND TEMPERATURE. Loss TO Supposed Weight Actual Loss Each 100 Gmsj . of Organs Before of Organs of Fresh Org/n^ I Starvation. in Gms. (Percentage TJo&b). Bone 393.4 54.7 13.9 / *> Muscle 1408.4 429.4 30.5 / ' Liver 91.9 49.4 53.7 Kidney 25.1 6.5 25.9 Spleen 8.7 5.8 66.7 1 Pancreas 6.5 1.1 17.0 .J . , . 4| s. Testes 2.5 1.0 40.0 \ Lungs 15.8 2.8 17.7 Heart 11.5 0.3 2.6 »^ Intestines 118.0 20.9 18.0 \< /\ A Brain and cord .... 40.7 1.3 3.2 \ ;> J / Skin and hair 432.8 89.3 20.6 VT Fat 275.4 267.2 97.0 Blood 138.5 37.3 27.0 Remainder 136.0 50.0 36.8 According to these results, the greatest absolute loss was in the muscles (429 gms.), while the greatest percentage loss was in the fat (97 per cent.), which had practically disappeared from the body. It is very significant that the central nervous system and the heart, organs which we may suppose were in continual activity, suffered practically no loss of weight : they had lived at the expense of the other tissues. We must suppose that in a starving animal the fat and the proteid materials, particularly in the voluntary muscles, pass into solution in the blood, and are then used to nourish the tissues generally and to supply the heat necessary to maintain the body temperature. Examination of the excreta in starving ani- mals has shown that a greater quantity of proteid is destroyed dur- ing the first day or two than in the subsequent days. This fact is explained on the supposition that the body is at first richly sup- plied with "circulating proteid" derived from its previous food, and that after this is metabolized the animal lives entirely, so far as proteid consumption is concerned, upon its "tissue proteid." If the animal remains quiet during starvation, the amount of nitrogen excreted daily soon reaches a nearly constant minimum, showing that a practically constant amount of proteid (together with fat) is consumed daily to furnish body heat, and probably to repair tissue waste in the active organs, such as the heart. Shortly before death from starvation the daily amount of proteid consumed may increase, as shown by the larger amount of nitrogen eliminated. This fact is explained by assuming that the body fat is then exhausted and the animal's metabolism is confined to the tissue proteids alone. The general fact that the loss of proteid is greatest during the first one or two days of starvation has been confirmed recently upon men, in a number of interesting experiments made upon professional f asters. For the numerous details as to loss of weight, variations of temperature, etc., carefully recorded in these latter experiments, 814 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. reference must be made to original sources.* It may be added, in conclusion, that the fatter the body is to begin with, the longer will starvation be endured, and if water is consumed freely the evil effects of starvation, as well as the disagreeable sensations of hun- ger, are very much reduced. The Potential Energy of Food. — The food material during digestion and after absorption undergoes numerous chemical changes in the body. Some of these changes are not attended by the libera- tion of heat to any marked extent. Such is the case, for instance, with the hydrolytic cleavages of the molecule which have been described especially in connection with the digestive processes. Similar hydrolytic cleavages occur doubtless within the tissues, and other changes connected with muscular, nervous, and glandular activity, and the building up and breaking down of the living sub- stance, take place constantly as a part of general nutritional metab- olism. On the other hand, many of the chemical processes occur- ring in the body are especially valuable on account of the heat liberated. These reactions, for the most part, at least, are oxida- tions; they are effected under the influence of oxidizing enzymes or by some other means of activating the oxygen. The various stages in the process are not explained, but we know that oxygen is neces- sary and that the carbon and the hydrogen contained in the sub- stances acted upon appear eventually in the form of oxidation prod- ucts— namely, carbon dioxid and water — Liebig designated the fats and carbohydrates as respiratory foods on the hypothesis that their fate in the body is to be oxidized and furnish heat. While this view is, in the main, correct, it is evident now that a portion at least of the proteid molecule, after the splitting off of the nitrogen, may also undergo oxidation and furnish heat. In Liebig 's sense, therefore, the proteids play the part of respiratory or heat-producing foods as well as acting as tissue formers. On the other hand, fats and carbohydrate material may enter to some extent, together with the proteid, into the synthesis of living material, and thus play the role of a plastic or tissue-forming as well as of a respiratory food. We can not divide the foodstuffs, therefore, strictly into two such classes, but we may perhaps consider the chemical processes in the body under the two heads mentioned above, — namely, the oxi- dation or heat-producing changes and those of general nutrition. The latter may give rise to heat or possibly absorb heat, but the great supply of heat energy needed by the body to maintain its temperature comes from the oxidation processes. This classifica- tion is employed by some physiologists, and is helpful in empha- sizing the fact that many chemical changes occur in the body that * "Virchow's Archiv, " vol. 131, supplement, 1893; and Luciani, "Das Hungern," 1890. See also Weber, " Ergebnisse der Physiologie, " vol. i, part i, 1902. HEAT ENERGY OF FOODS. 815 are of no importance from the standpoint of heat production, and that the changes that do give rise mainly to heat form, as it were, a special group, which is not connected with the building up or breaking down of the living matter, but furnishes the energy by means of which these latter changes and perhaps other functions, such as muscular work, are made possible. Roughly speaking, an adult man forms in his body and gives off to the surrounding air about 2,400,000 calories of heat per day. By calorie or small calorie (c) is meant the quantity of heat necessary to raise 1 gm. of water 1° in temperature. This great supply of heat is derived from the physiological oxidation of the carbohydrate, fat, and proteid material of the food. These same materials may be oxi- dized outside the body by burning them at a high temperature or under a high pressure of oxygen, and the heat that they give off in the process can be measured directly. So far as the fats and carbo- hydrates are concerned, the end-products of the oxidation in the body are the same as in their combustion out of the body, and we may believe, therefore, that the amount of heat produced is the same in both cases. Consequently the heat value of a gram of fat or carbohydrate burnt outside the body is spoken of as its combus- tion equivalent, and it measures the amount of potential energy of these foodstuffs with regard to their capacity for the production of heat or of muscular work in the body. With regard to the pro- teid, the case is somewhat different. Its end-products in the body are carbon dioxid, water, and urea or some other of the nitrogenous waste products. These nitrogenous wastes are capable of further oxidation with liberation of heat, so that, as far as they are elimi- nated, the body loses a possible supply of heat energy, which must be subtracted from the total heat energy that the proteid gives upon oxidation outside the body, in order to determine the available heat energy yielded within the body. The figures obtained for the heat equivalents of the foodstuffs by burning them outside the body in some form of calorimeter are as follows : 1 gm. of fat yields an aver- age of 9300 calories, or 9.3 large calories (C), 1 gm. of carbohydrate yields an average of 4100 calories (4.1 C). These figures may be taken, therefore, to express the quantity of heat given to the body by the oxidation within its tissues of these elements of our food. A gram of proteid when burnt outside the body yields on the aver- age 5778 calories. The heat value of the urea is estimated as 1 gm. = 2523 calories. If we assume that all the nitrogen of the pro- teid appears as urea and that 1 gm. of proteid yields J gm. of urea, then the available heat energy of a gram of proteid should be equal to 5778 — 841 (or J of 2523) = 4937 calories. Later workers, however, have given reasons for believing that this last figure is too high. All of the nitrogen is not eliminated as urea, and, moreover, all of the nitrogenous waste is not excreted in the urine; a distinct pro- 816 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. portion is given off in the feces. Rubner has calculated the avail- able heat energy of proteids by direct experiments upon animals. In these experiments the heat value of the proteid fed was directly determined by burning a sample in a calorimeter. Then after feed- ing a known amount of the proteid the urine and feces were col- lected and their heat value was determined in the same way. The difference between the total heat value of the proteid fed and the heat value lost in its excreted products in the feces and urine gave the actual heat energy obtained from the proteid by the ani- mal body. Results obtained by this method give an average value for 1 gm. proteid of 4100 calories (4.1 C), or, since proteid contains an average of 16 per cent, of nitrogen, we may say that 1 gm. of ni- trogen ingested as proteid has a heat value of 4.1 X 6.25 = 25.6 C. The figures that are used, therefore, in estimating the heat value of our foodstuffs are: 1 gm. proteid = 4100 calories (4.1 C). 1 gm. carbohydrate = 4100 calories (4.1 C). 1 gm. fat = 9305 calories (9.3 C). Making use of these values, it is obvious that we can calculate the total heat value of any given diet. If we analyze the food for its composition in the three principal foodstuffs we may determine how many calories will be furnished to the body. In many of the tables published to show the composition of the different foods figures are given also to express their heat value or potential energy, on the belief that, for the most part, our food is used as fuel to supply energy to the body. These values for some of our ordinary foods are as follows : * Proteid. Fat. Beefsteak, porterhouse 19.1 17.9 Beefsteak, round (lean) 20.2 2.4 Corned beef (canned) 26.3 18.7 Veal, leg (lean) 19.4 3.7 Veal liver 19.0 5.3 Mutton, leg (lean) • 16.5 10.3 Pork, ham (fresh, lean) 24.8 14.2 Pork chops, medium fat 13.4 24.2 Chicken (fowl) 13.7 12.3 Shad 9.4 4.8 Shad roe 20.9 3.8 Eggs 11.7 10.7 Milk 3.3 4.0 Oatmeal 16.1 7.2 Rice 8.0 0.3 Wheat flour (entire wheat) 13.8 1.9 Green peas 7.0 0.5 Potatoes (raw) 2.2 0.1 Spinach 2.1 0.3 Tomatoes... 0.9 0.4 Apples 0.4 0.5 Bananas 1.3 0.6 Carbohy- drate. 2.6 5.0 67.5 79.0 71.9 16.9 18.4 3.2 3.9 14.2 Ash. 0.8 1.2 4.0 1.1 1.3 0.9 1.3 0.8 o:7 0.7 1.5 0.7 0.7 1.9 0.4 1.0 1.0 1.0 2.1 0.5 0.3 0.8 Heat Value in Calories Per Pound. 1110 475 1280 520 575 740 1060 1270 775 380 600 680 325 1860 1630 1675 465 385 110 105 * 290 460 22.0 * Selected from Atwater and Bryant, Bulletin 28 (revised edition), United States Department of Agriculture, 1899. DIETETICS. 817 It must be borne in mind, however, that the entire nutritional value of a food is not expressed in its heat value — some of our food mate- rial— the green foods and fruits, for example — are useful and in a measure essential because of their salts and organic acids. In a general way, however, the heat energy of a food expresses its value as a means for maintaining the body in a normal condition. Dietetics. — The subject of the proper nourishment of individ- uals or collection of individuals in health and in sickness is treated usually in works upon hygiene or dietetics. The practical details of the preparation and composition of diets must be obtained from such sources.* The general principles upon which practical diet- ing depends have been obtained, however, from experimental work upon the nutrition of man and the lower animals, some account of which has been given in the foregoing pages. In a healthy adult the main objects of a diet are to furnish sufficient nitrogenous and non- nitrogenous foodstuffs, salts, and water to maintain the body in equi- librium of material and of energy — that is, the diet must furnish the material for the regeneration of tissue, and the material for the heat produced and the muscular work done. Nutritional experiments prove that this object may be accomplished by proteid food alone together with salts and water. It is doubtful, however, whether, in the case of man, such a diet could be continued for long periods without causing some nutritional disturbance, directly or indi- rectly. It will be remembered that a pure meat diet is not entirely proteid, since all flesh contains some fats and carbohydrates, (glyco- gen). The functions of a diet are accomplished more easily and more economically when it is composed of proteids and fats, or pro- teids and carbohydrates, or, as is almost universally the case, of proteids, fats, and carbohydrates. The experience of mankind shows that such a mixed diet is most beneficial to the body and most satisfying to that valuable regulating mechanism of nutrition, the appetite. The proportions in which the proteids, fats, and carbohydrates are mixed in a diet vary greatly among different nations and individuals. So far as the fats and carbohydrates are concerned, their use is mainly that of fuel to supply energy, and from this standpoint we ought to be able to exchange them in the diet in the ratio of their heat values. This ratio, or as it is frequently called, the isodynamic equiva- lent, is 'as 9.3 to 4.1 or 2.3 to 1, and within the limits permitted by the appetite we should be able to substitute 1 part of fat for 2.3 parts of sugar or starch. Experiments upon animals as well as the experience of mankind show that this substitution can be made. * For practical directions see Gautier, " L'alimentation et les regimes," 1904; Blyth, "Foods : their Composition and Analysis." 52 818 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. This fact is illustrated in a general way by the different diets recom- mended by various physiologists, as follows : AVERAGE DIETS AND THEIR HEAT VALUES. MOLESCHOTT. RaNKE. VoiT. Calories. Calories. Calories. Proteid 130 gms . . . 533,000 100 gms, . . . 410,000 118 gms. . . . 483,000 Fats 40 "' ... 372,000 100 " ... 930,000 56 " ... 520,800 Carbohydrates. 550 " . ..2,275,000 240 " . . . 984,000 500 " ...2,050,000 2,980,000 2,324,000 3,053,800 FORSTER. ATWATER. Calories. Calories. Proteid 131 gins. . . . 567,100 125 gms. . . . 512,500 Fats 68 " ... 632,400 125 " ...1,172,500 Carbohydrates. 494 " . ..1,825,400 400 " ■ ..1,640,000 2,024,900 3,325,000 The average heat value of these diets is equal to 2,741,540 calo- ries. In round numbers it is usually estimated that the diet should furnish daily 2,400,000 calories for an individual weighing 60 kgms., or about 40,000 calories per kgm. of body-weight. It will be noticed that in all cases the greatest portion of this energy is obtained from the carbohydrate food, which, on account of its economy, its abun- dance, and its ease of digestion and oxidation in the body, consti- tutes the bulk of our diet. In cases of excessive muscular work the food eaten may supply more than twice the average heat value given above. Thus, Atwater and Sherman * estimate that in a six- day bicycle race by professionals the heat value of the food for the different participants varied from 4,770,000 to 6,095,000 calories. Chittenden, in the work previously referred to,f has raised the question whether the heat value of the diet ordinarily employed is unnecessarily high. In his own case he found that the body could be well nourished on a diet containing a total heat value of only 1,600,000 calories or 28,000 calories per kgm. of body -weight instead of 40,000 calories. The diet in this case, it will be remem- bered, contained only 36 to 40 gms. of proteid in place of the 100 to 130 gms. recommended in the diets mentioned above. The ques- tion thus raised is one that must be decided by actual experience. Mankind is guided and has been guided in all times by the control of the appetite, and if scientific experiments indicate that this regulatory apparatus leads us to ingest more food than is actually required for the machinery of the body it remains for observation and experiment to determine whether this excess is beneficial or useless or perhaps even harmful. Munk gives an interesting table showing how much of certain * Bulletin 98, United States Department of Agriculture, 1901. t Chittenden, " Physiological Economy in Nutrition, " 1905. DIETETICS. 819 familiar articles of food would be necessary, if taken alone, to supply the requisite daily amount of proteid or non-proteid food ; his esti- mates are based upon the percentage composition of the foods and upon experimental data snowing the extent of absorption of the foodstuffs in each food. In this table he supposes that the daily diet should contain 110 gms. of proteid=17.5 gms. of N, and non- proteids sufficient to contain 270 gms. of C: For 110 Gms. Proteid (17.5 Gms. N). Milk 2900 gms. Meat (lean) 540 " Hen's eggs 18 eggs. Wheat flour 800 gms. Wheat bread 1650 " Rye bread 1900 " Rice 1870 " Corn 990 " Peas 520 " Potatoes 4500 " As Munk points out, this table shows that any single food, if taken in quantities sufficient to supply the nitrogen, would give too much or too little carbon and the reverse; those animal foods which, in certain amounts, supply the nitrogen needed furnish only from one- fourth to two-thirds of the necessary amount of carbon. To live for a stated period upon a single article of food — a diet sometimes recommended to reduce obesity — means, then, an insufficient quan- tity of either nitrogen or carbon and a consequent loss of body weight. Such a method of dieting amounts practically to a partial starvation. In practical dieting we are accustomed to get our supply of proteids, fats, and carbohydrates from both vegetable and animal foods. To illustrate this fact by an actual case, in which the food was carefully analyzed, an experimenter weighing 67 kgms. records that he kept himself in nitrogen equilibrium upon a diet in which the proteid was distributed as follows: For 270 Gms. C. 3800 2000 gms. u 37 670 1000 eggs, gms. u 1100 u 750 (I 660 a 750 (i 2550 t( 300 gms. meat = 63.08 gms. proteid = 9.78 gms. N 666.3 c.c. milk = 18.74 " a = 2.905 " " 100 gms. rice = 7.74 " a = 1.2 " " 100 " bread = 11.32 " a = 1.755 " " 500 c.c. wine 1.17 " 0.182 gm. " 102.05 " 15.868 gms. " For a person in health and leading an active, normal life, appetite and experience seem to be safe and sufficient guides by which to control the diet; but in conditions of disease, in regulating the diet of children and of collections of individuals, scientific dieting, if one may use the phrase, has accomplished much, and will be of greater service as our knowledge of the physiology of nutrition increases. CHAPTER LI. THE PRODUCTION OF HEAT IN THE BODY— ITS MEAS- UREMENT AND REGULATION— BODY TEMPERA- TURE— CALORIMETRY— PHYSIOLOGICAL OXIDATIONS. It is customary to date our modern ideas of the origin of animal heat from the time of Lavoisier (1774-77). To the older physiologists it was a most difficult problem. The animal's body produces heat continually and maintains a temperature higher, as a rule, than that of the surrounding air. Since oxygen and the nature of ordinary combustions were unknown, they naturally explained this heat formation by reference to causes which the science of the day had shown to be capable of producing warmth, such as friction and fermentation. Haller (1757), for instance, taught that the body heat arises mainly from the friction of the circulating blood and the movements of the heart and blood-vessels, and this view found currency in text-books well into the nine- teenth century. Lavoisier first gave to the physiologist the con- ception that the heat produced in the body is due to a combustion or oxidation, and that therein lies the significance of our respiration of oxygen. He believed himself that this oxidation takes place in the lungs, — that is, the blood brings to the lungs a hydrocarbon- ous material which is attacked by the oxygen and burnt with the formation of water and carbon dioxid and the liberation of heat. Later experimenters demonstrated that the heat production does not occur in the lungs, at least not exclusively, but over the whole of the body. After a long and interesting controversy it was also shown satisfactorily that the oxidations of the body do not occur in the blood, but in the tissues themselves. The oxygen is transported to the cells and there does its work of effecting oxi- dations and giving rise to heat. This heat is equalized more or less over the whole body, chiefly by the circulation of the blood, which absorbs heat from the warmer organs and distributes it to the cooler ones. The body temperature is maintained at a nearly constant level by an intricate adjustment of physiological reflexes which together constitute the heat-regulating mechanism. Such in brief is the general theory of our time regarding heat production in the body. Many of the problems that interested the older phys- 820 BODY TEMPERATURE. 821 iologists have been solved satisfactorily, but there remain, of course, many more to interest this and succeeding generations. Investi- gations in this field at present are directed mainly to an effort to understand the details of the heat-regulating apparatus, on the one hand, and, on the other, to comprehend more satisfactorily the nature of the process of oxidation. This latter problem is one of common interest at present in chemistry and in physiology. The Body Temperature. — We divide animals into the two great classes of warm blooded and cold blooded, according as their temperature is or is not above that of the surrounding air. In this sense, birds and mammals are warm blooded and reptiles, amphibia, and fishes are cold blooded. The names, however, are badly chosen. The difference of deepest significance between the mammals and birds, on the one hand, and the fishes, amphibia, and reptiles, on the other, is that in the former the body temperature is, within wide limits, independent of the outside temperature; it remains practically constant during winter and summer, whether the surrounding air is hotter or cooler than the body. They are, therefore, constant-temperature animals (homoiothermous). The reptiles, amphibia, and fishes, on the contrary, have a body tem- perature that changes with the environment. On winter days their temperature is low, approximately that of the surrounding air or water, and in summer their body temperature rises to cor- respond with that of the outside. Strictly speaking, they are cold blooded only in cold surroundings. This group may be designated as the changeable- temperature animals (poikilothermous). The warm-blooded animals maintain a constant high body temperature because their oxidations are more rapid and they possess a heat- regulating mechanism. In the cold-blooded animals the oxidations are not so intense, and a heat-regulating mechanism is absent or poorly developed. The hibernating animals form a group inter- mediate in many ways between these two classes. They possess a heat-regulating apparatus that maintains a constant body tem- perature under most conditions, but breaks down in very cold weather; so that during the period of winter sleep their tem- perature is but little above that of the surrounding air. In some of the cold-blooded animals the production of heat is more rapid during warm weather than its loss; so that they exhibit a body temperature slightly higher than the surrounding me- dium. A hive of bees in activity may raise the temperature within the hive through a number of degrees and snakes and many reptiles show a temperature of 2° to 8° C. above that of the air. So also some reptiles possess a rudimentary means of protecting their bodies from too great a rise of temperature, — for instance, by accelerated breathing whereby more water is evap- 822 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. orated from the lungs and thus more heat is lost.* The distinc- tion between the two great groups of animals is not entirely abso- lute, but it is sufficiently marked to constitute a striking physio- logical characteristic. The temperature of the human body is measured usually by thermometers placed in the mouth, in the axilla, or in the rectum. Measurements made in this way show that in general the tempera- ture in the interior of the body (rectal) is slightly higher than on the surface of the skin. The average temperature in the rectum is 37.2° C. (98.96° F.); in the axilla, 36.9° C. (98.45° F.); in the mouth, 36.87° C. (98.36° F.). We may speak of the body temperature, therefore, in the places in which it can be conveniently measured, as varying between 36.87° C. and 37.2° C. Some of the internal or- gans have a higher temperature, particularly during their period of greatest activity. The temperature of man, measured in the places mentioned, shows also a distinct variation during the day, a diurnal rhythm. This daily variation has been measured by many ob- servers, and shows individual peculiarities that depend largely upon the manner of living, time of meals, etc. In general it may be said that the lowest temperature is shown early in the morning, — 6 to 7 a.m. ; that it rises slowly during the day to reach its maximum in the evening, 5 to 7 p.m. ; and falls again during the night. The difference between early morning and late afternoon or evening may amount to a degree or more centigrade, and this fact must be borne in mind by physicians when observing the temperature of patients. Muscular activity and food appear to be the factors that are mainly responsible for the rise in temperature during the day. Most observers state that when the habits of life are reversed for some time — that is, when work is performed and meals are eaten during the night, and the day is given up to sleep and rest — the daily variation of temperature is inverted to correspond, — that is, the highest temperature is observed in the early morning and the lowest in the late afternoon. Age also has a slight influence. Newly born infants and young children have a somewhat higher temperature than adults. The difference may amount to half a degree or a degree centigrade, — 37.6° C. in infants as compared with 36.6° C. or 37. 1° C. in the adult. It is known, also, that the heat- regulating mechanism in infants and young children is not so efficient as in adults, and that therefore febrile disturbances are more easily excited in the former than in the latter. In the matter of body temperature, as in so many other characteristics, aged people show a tendency to revert to infantile conditions. Their temperature, according to most observers, is slightly higher than in middle life. * See Langlois, " Journal de physiologie et de pathol. generate, " 1902, 249. CALORIMETRY. 823 Among physiological conditions that influence the body tempera- ture, muscular work and meals, as stated above, have the most posi- tive effect. Marked muscular activity implies a great increase in the production of heat in the body and most observers find that the initial result at least is a small rise in body temperature, — a fact which indicates that the heat regulation is not perfect; the excess of heat produced is not dissipated promptly. In the period of rest following upon work, on the contrary, the body temperature may fall, owing probably to the fact that more heat is lost through the flushed skin than is produced within the body. In this matter of the effect of muscular work individual variations are to be expected, since the perfection of the heat-regulating mechanisms may vary somewhat in different persons. Meals also cause a slight rise in body temperature, which reaches its maximum about an hour and a half after the ingestion of the food. The explanation in this case also is to be found doubtless in a greater production of heat, due to the increased metabolism in the secreting glands, the liver, and the musculature of the gastro-intestinal canal. The excessive production of heat is not compensated completely by a corresponding increase in the heat dissipated.* It is sufficiently obvious, perhaps, from these facts that the temperature as measured by the thermometer is a balance between the amount of heat produced and the amount of heat lost or dissipated. The thermometer alone gives us no cer- tain indication of the quanity of heat produced in the body. A temperature higher than normal, fever temperature, may be due either to an excessive production of heat or to a deficient dissipa- tion. To understand and control the processes by which the body temperature is kept normal it is necessary to discover a means for ascertaining at any time the actual quantities of heat produced and dissipated, and the effect upon each factor of different normal and pathological conditions. The means used for this purpose is the method of calorimetry. It is necessary, therefore, to describe the principle and construction of calorimeters and the methods of calorimetry before attempting to explain the mechanism of heat regulation. Calorimetry. — A calorimeter is an instrument for measuring the quantity of heat given off from a body. The unit employed in these determinations is the calorie, — that is, the amount of heat necessary to raise 1 gm. of water 1° C.,or more accurately the amount of heat required to raise 1 gm. of water from 15° to 16° C. This unit is sometimes designated as a small calorie to distinguish it from the large calorie (C), — that is, the quantity of heat necessary to raise 1 kgm. of water 1° C. The large calorie is equal to 1000 * For further details see Richet, " La chaleur animate, " 1889 ; and Pem- brey, "Animal Heat," Schaefer's "Text-book of Physiology, " vol. i, 1898. 824 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. small calories. In physiology calorimeters have been used for two main purposes: to determine the heat equivalent of foods, — that is, the amount of heat given off when the various foodstuffs are burned, — and, secondly, to determine the heat produced and the heat dissi- pated by living animals during a given period. For the first pur- pose the apparatus that is most frequently employed at present is the bomb calorimeter devised by Berthelot. The bomb consists of a strong steel cylinder in which the food to be burned is placed and which is filled with oxygen under high pressure. The combus- tion of the foodstuff is initiated by means of a spiral of platinum CT\ \ENT Fig. 266. — Reichert's water calorimeter. wire heated by an electrical current. The bomb is immersed in water and the heat given off raises the water to a measured extent of temperature. The weight of water being known, the amount of heat is easily expressed in calories. For the purpose of measuring the heat given off by living animals two principal forms of calorim- eter are used, each form having a number of modifications. These two forms are the water calorimeter and the air calorimeter. The water calorimeter was the form used in the first experiments on rec- ord (Crawford, 1779). In principle it consists of a double-walled box with a known weight of water between the walls. The animal CALORIMETRY. 825 is placed in the inner box and the heat given off is absorbed by the water. Knowing the weight of the water and how much its tem- perature is raised, the data are at hand for determining the number of calories given off during the experiment. A recent form of this variety of calorimeter used in this country by Reichert is shown in Fig. 266. It consists of two concentric boxes of metal with a space between them of about one and a half inches. The animal is placed in the inner box (A). The two boxes are inclosed in a large wooden box, the space between the metal and wooden boxes being filled with shavings (SH). The object of this outer box is to prevent radiation of heat from the metal boxes. The tubes EN and EX, which lead into the interior chamber containing the animal, are for the entrance and exit of the ventilating air. A thermometer is placed in each to determine the heat carried off by the air. The thermometer, CT, measures the temperature of the water, and $ is a stirrer to keep the water well mixed and thus insure a uniform temperature. When the animal is placed in the apparatus the heat given off warms not only the water, but also the metal; so that to determine the total heat the weight of metal must be re- duced to an equivalent amount of water by multiplying its weight by its specific heat, or, a more simple method, the calorimetric equiv- alent of the apparatus is determined, — that is, the actual amount of heat necessary to raise the temperature of the apparatus, water and metal, one degree. This value is obtained by burning in the appa- ratus a known weight of some substance (alcohol, hydrogen) whose heat of combustion is known. Knowing how much heat is given off by this combustion and how much the temperature of the apparatus is raised, the calorimetric equivalent is easily calcu- lated and may be used subsequently in estimating the results ob- tained from animals. In the use of the apparatus many precau- tions must be observed. These practical details need not be des- cribed here except to say that account must be taken of the warm- ing of the air used to ventilate the apparatus and of any changes in the amount of its moisture. The calorimeter used in this way measures directly the amount of heat given off from the animal during the period of observation. The amount of heat produced in the animal's body during this time may be the same, or may be more or less. To arrive at a knowledge of this factor observations must be made upon the animal's body temperature by means of a thermometer in the rectum. If this body temperature is the same at the end as at the beginning of the experiment then it is obvious that the heat produced must have been equal to the heat lost. If the animal's body temperature has fallen, then it is evident that less heat has been produced than was lost. To ascertain how much less, the weight of the animal is multiplied by its specific heat (0 . 8) 826 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. to reduce it to so much water, and this product is multiplied by the difference in body temperature at the beginning and the end of the experiment. The product is obtained in calories and is subtracted from the amount of heat lost, as determined by the calorimeter, to obtain the amount of heat produced. If, on the contrary, the ani- mal's temperature has risen during the experiment the body has produced more heat than it has dissipated. The increase may be determined as above by multiplying the weight of the animal, the specific heat of the body, and the difference in temperature. This amount added to the heat lost gives the heat produced. Most recent investigators have used some form of air calorimeter. An air calorimeter consists essentially of a double-walled chamber or box with air between the walls. The animal is placed in the inner box and the heat given off is measured by the expansion of the air between the walls. Many different forms are used, prefer- iHf=n Fig. 267. — D'Arsonval's differential calorimeter. ence being given to some modification of the differential air calo- rimeter. In this last-named instrument two exactly similar chambers are constructed ; one contains the animal while the other serves as a dummy. These two chambers are balanced against each other, the air space in the dummy being heated by immersion in a bath or by burning hydrogen in the interior. As these sources of heat are known and can be controlled, it is evident that if the dummy is made to balance exactly the chamber containing the animal the amount of heat given off in each is the same. The principle of the differential calorimeter is represented in Fig. 267, which gives a schema of the form originally employed by d'Arsonval; 8 and 8' represent the two calorimeters, in one of which the animal is placed while the other acts as dummy. Each is double walled and the air spaces are connected by tubes, 10 and 10', to small gasometers, 4, 4', suspended in water and hung on opposite sides of a balance. The movements of these gasometers antagonize each other, and the CALORIMETRY. 827 resultant may be recorded upon smoked paper, as indicated in the figure.* The Respiration Calorimeter. — When a calorimeter is so arranged that the composition of the air drawn through the apparatus for ventilation can be determined as well as the amount of heat pro- duced, the apparatus becomes a respiration calorimeter. In such an apparatus, if proper provision is made for analyzing the urine, the feces, and the food, the total carbon and nitrogen excretion may be obtained simultaneously with the heat loss. Since we may calculate from the carbon and nitrogen excretion how much pro- teid, fat, and carbohydrate have been burnt in the body, and since the heat values of these constituents are known, it is evident that we may reckon indirectly how much heat ought to be produced from the combustion of so much material. This method of arriv- ing at the heat production is designated indirect calorimetry. With an adequate respiration calorimeter it is possible to ascertain whether the results calculated by the method of indirect calorim- etry really correspond with the heat obtained by direct measure- ment. In the hands of good observers the correspondence is very close, and gives substantial proof of the scientific belief that in the living body the energy liberated as heat or as heat and work is all contained in potential form in the foodstuffs eaten. By means of the respiration calorimeter we can obtain a balance between the energy income and outgo of the body as well as between the material income and outgo, — that is, the carbon and nitrogen equilibrium. The most complete and elaborate form of respiration calorimeter used is that devised by Atwater and Rosa for experiments upon man.f By means of this apparatus many inter- esting and important experiments have been made upon the nu- trition of man under special conditions. Such results as the follow- ing have been obtained (Atwater and Benedict) in the case of a man who, while in the apparatus, did much muscular work on a bicycle ergometer: Income : Potential energy of material metabolized in body = 5459 Cal. Outeo •! Energy given off from the body as heat .... 4833 Cal. g \ Heat equivalent of muscular work 602 Cal. 5435 Cal. 5435 Cal. Experimental error 24 Cal. * For detailed accounts of special forms of air calorimeters see Rubner, u Calorimetrische Methodik," 1891; and Rosenthal, "Archiv f. Physiol- ogie, " 1897, p. 170. f See Atwater and Rosa, Bulletin 63, United States Department of Agri- culture, 1899; and for recent improvements, Atwater and Benedict, 1905. 828 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. Results of Calorimetric Measurements. — The actual results obtained from direct calorimetric measurements corroborate those deduced from the study of the energy given off in the oxidation of the foodstuffs of the daily diet. They show that man gives off heat from his body to the amount of 40,000 to 50,000 calories per kgm. of weight during 24 hours under conditions of ordinary life, — a total, therefore, of 2,400,000 to 3,000,000 calories per day for an individual weighing 60 kgms. This amount is increased greatly under conditions demanding much muscular work. This loss of heat is, of course, made good by the production of an equal amount within the body by the oxidation of the food material. Actual experiments upon different animals* show that small animals produce more heat in proportion to their weight than larger animals of the same species, owing to their relatively larger surface and therefore greater loss of heat. Birds produce and lose more heat for a unit of surface than mammals, — a fact which indicates that their physiological oxidations are more intense. According to Richet, a sparrow gives off per unit of surface five times as much heat as a rabbit. According to Rubner, the sparrow produces thirteen times as much heat as man for the same amount of tissue. In infants, owing to their larger surface relative to the mass of the body, the loss of heat is greater than in the adult. HEAT REGULATION. From a general standpoint the most important problem that the physiologist has to study is the means by which the heat production and heat loss are so regulated as to maintain a practically constant body temperature. Experiments show that the mechanism of heat regulation is very complex and is two-sided, — that is, the body possesses means of controlling the loss of heat as well as the produc- tion of heat, and under the conditions of normal life both means are used. Regulation of the Heat Loss. — Heat is regularly lost from our bodies in a number of different ways, which may be classified as' follows : 1. Through the excreta, urine, feces, saliva, which are at the temperature of the body when voided. 2. Through the expired air. This air is warmer than the inspired air, and moreover is nearly saturated with water-vapor. The vaporiza- tion of water requires heat, which is, of course, taken from the body supply. Each gram of water requires for its vaporization about 582 calories. 3. By evaporation of the sweat from the skin. The amount lost in this way increases naturally with the amount of sweat secreted. 4. By conduction and especially by radiation of heat from the skin. *See Rubner, " Zeitschrift. f. Biologie," 19, 535, 1883; and Richet, " La chaleur animale," 1889, p. 224. REGULATION OF THE HEAT LOSS. 829 The relative values of these different means of heat loss are estimated as follows by Vierordt: 1. By urine and feces 1.8 per cent, or 47,500 calories. 2. By expired air: Warming of air 3.5 " " 84,500 Vaporization of water from lungs 7.2 " " 182,120 3. By evaporation from skin 14.5 " " 364,120 4. By radiation and conduction from skin .73.0 " " 1,791,820 Total daily loss = 2,500,000 In man this loss of heat is regulated chiefly by controlling the im- portant factors 3 and 4. We accomplish this end in part deliber- ately or voluntarily by the use of appropriate clothing. Clothing of any kind captures a layer of warm and moist air between it and the skin and thus diminishes greatly the loss by evaporation and by radiation. In cold weather the amount and character of the clothing is changed in order to diminish the heat loss. The ideal clothing for this purpose is made of material, such as wool, which, while porous enough to permit adequate ventilation of the air next to the skin, is at the same time a poor conductor of heat and thus diminishes the main factor of loss by radiation. The most impor- tant means of controlling the heat loss, however, is by automatic reflex control through the sweat nerves and the vasomotor nerves. In warm weather the secretion of sweat is greatly increased by re- flex stimulation of the sweat nerves. The greater amount of water requires a greater amount of heat to vaporize it, and thus the heat loss is increased. The value of this control is illustrated by a case recorded by Zuntz * of a man who possessed no sweat glands. In summer this individual was incapacitated for work, since even a small degree of muscular activity would cause an increase in his body temperature to 40° or 41° C. The control through the vasomotor nerves is doubtless even more important. The blood-vessels bring the warm blood to the skin, where it loses its heat by conduction and especially by radia- tion to the cooler air. When the surrounding air is much below the temperature of the body the vasoconstrictor center is stimulated, the blood-vessels in the skin are constricted, the supply of warm blood to the skin is diminished, and therefore the amount of heat lost is less. The reflex in this case may be attributed primarily to the action of the cool air on the cold nerves of the skin. The impulses carried by these fibers to the nerve centers stimulate the vasocon- strictor center or that part of it controlling the vasomotor fibers to the skin. On warm days, on the contrary, the blood-vessels in the skin are dilated sometimes to an extreme extent, the supply of warm blood is therefore increased, and more heat is lost if the air is lower in temperature than the blood. The * Zuntz, " Deutsche medizinal-Zeitung, " 1903, No. 25. 830 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. reflex in this case may be regarded possibly as an inhibition of the vasoconstrictor center through the warm nerves of the skin. Sub- stances, such as alcohol, which cause a dilatation of the skin ves- sels also increase the loss of body heat, in some cases to a sufficient extent to lower the body temperature. To a smaller extent our heat loss is controlled through an acceleration of the breathing movements. The greatly increased respirations in muscular ac- tivity must aid somewhat in eliminating the excess of heat produced, although this factor must be much less important than the sweating and the flushing of the skin which are produced reflexly during muscular work. In some of the lower animals — the dog, for in- stance— in which the sweat nerves are absent over most of the body and in which the coat of hair interferes with the free loss by radiation, it is found that the loss through the respiratory channel is relatively more important. The panting of the dog is a familiar phenomenon. Richet has studied this reflex upon dogs and has designated the greatly accelerated breathing in warm weather or after muscular exercise as thermic polypnea (according to Gad, tachypnea). He assumes a special center for the reflex situated in the medulla and acting through the respiratory center. It is a curious fact, as shown by Langlois, that some reptiles exhibit a similar reflex; when their body temperature is raised to 39° C. they show a condition of marked polypnea (rapid breathing) the ap- parent object of which is to augment the loss of heat from the body. Regulation of Heat Production. — Heat production is varied in the body by increasing or decreasing the physiological oxida- tions. This end is effected in part voluntarily by muscular exercise or by taking more food. Muscular contractions are attended by a marked liberation of heat and it is a part of everyone's experience that by work or muscular activity the effect of outside cold may be counteracted. In the case of food the body burns promptly most of the material of a daily diet. By increasing the diet in cold weather provision is made for the greater supply of heat required. In normal individuals this regulation is not, strictly speaking, voluntary. Outside cold is most effective in stimulating the appe- tite and thus leading us to increase the diet. In this, as in other respects, the appetite serves to control the amount of food in pro- portion to the needs of the body. The purely involuntary control of heat production consists of an involuntary reflex upon muscu- lar metabolism and possibly in the existence of a special set of heat centers and heat nerves. With regard to the first effect we have the striking experiments quoted by Pfliiger,* according to which a rabbit paralyzed by large doses of curare is no longer able to main- * Pfliiger, " Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 18, 255, 1878. REGULATION OF HEAT PRODUCTION. 831 tain its body temperature when the outside temperature is changed. The rabbit behaves, in fact, like a cold-blooded animal. In the calorimeter it shows a marked loss of. heat production, and its temperature may be made to go up and down with the outside temperature. The same result may be obtained by section of all the motor nerves, — that is, section of the spinal cord in the upper cervical region. Rubner has shown by calorimetric experiments upon normal animals (dogs) that the body temperature, as we know, remains constant when the outside temperature is changed, but that the heat production is increased as the outside tempera- ture is lowered. Finally, Johannson* has shown that the increased oxidations that occur under the influence of outside cold, as meas- ured by the C02 output, occur only when muscular tension is increased or shivering is noticed. We may believe, therefore, that the increased oxidations caused by cold are due to motor reflexes upon the skeletal muscles. These reflexes take place doubtless through the motor fibers, and lead to an augmented muscular tone or to small contractions (shivering) according to their inten- sity. This fact accords with one's personal sensations regarding the condition of his muscles in cold weather. The Existence of Heat Centers and Heat Nerves. — Physi- ologists have long supposed that there may be in the body a special set of heat nerves and heat centers, separate in their action from the motor, secretory, and other efferent nerves that influence the me- tabolism of the peripheral organs. It is supposed that these fibers, if they exist, when in activity augment or inhibit the physiological oxidations in the tissues, and that this effect has for its specific object an increase or decrease in heat production, .outside of any functional activity of the tissues. Bernard thought at first that he had demonstrated the existence of calorific fibers in the cervical sympathetic, but it was afterward recognized that the fibers in question are vasoconstrictors. Since that time very numerous experiments have been made with this object in view, but it must be admitted that no conclusive proof has yet been obtained of the existence of such a system. The evidence that has been most re- lied upon is the effect of lesions, experimental or pathological, of definite portions of the brain or cord. The following facts are significant: A number of observers f have found that section or puncture of the brain at the junction of medulla and pons causes an increase in heat production and a rise of temperature. Section of the cord in the cervical region is, on the other hand, attended usually by a fall in body temperature. These experiments might be * Johannson, " Skandinavisches Archiv f. Physiologie, " 7, 123, 1897. t See Wood, " Fever, " " Smithsonian Contributions to Knowledge. " Washington, 1880. 832 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. interpreted to mean that there exists in the brain anterior to the medulla a general heat center of an inhibitory character. Under normal conditions this center may hold the lower heat-producing centers in check. When cut off by section this inhibitory influence is removed and increase in heat production and body temperature results. A second important fact, brought out by Ott,* is that in- jury to the corpus striatum causes a rise in heat production and body temperature. This result has been confirmed by many other investigators, making use especially of what is known as the " heat puncture." In this experiment, made upon rabbits, a probe or style is inserted into the brain so as to puncture the corpus stria- tum. The result in the majority of cases is a rise of temperature which may last for a long time, although the animal shows no par- alysis and apparently no other effect from the operation. Accord- ing to some observers, t the increased production of heat takes place mainly in the liver, and is due to the oxidation of the glycogen. According to others (Aronsohn), the increased production of heat occurs mainly in the muscles. The fever produced by the " heat puncture'' seems to be due essentially to an irritation of the nerv- ous system, and is an experimental demonstration of the possi- bility of fever arising from lesions of the nerve centers. White and others have described similar disturbances of heat production from lesions of the optic thalamus. Heat centers have been located also in the septum lucidum, in the cortex, the midbrain, pons, and medulla, while Reichert places the primary heat-producing centers (thermogenic centers), from which the hypothetical heat nerves originate directly, in the spinal cord in the anterior horn of the gray matter .J The great amount of experimental work done along these lines has been inspired doubtless by the hope of discovering a special heat-regulating nervous apparatus which if demonstrated would enable us to explain the causation of fevers. In its most elaborate form this hypothesis assumes the existence of primary heat-producing (thermogenic) centers in the cord and brain from which the calorific or heat nerves arise. These centers in turn are controlled by regulating (thermotaxic) centers of an augmenting •and inhibitory character in the higher portions of the brain. By reflex influences upon these latter centers the activity of the thermo- genic centers may be increased or diminished and the production of heat in the body controlled. While such an apparatus may exist, it is nevertheless true that the evidence in favor of it so far *Ott, "Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases," 1884, 1887, 1888; also "Brain," 1889. t Roily, " Deutsches Archiv f . klinische Medicin, " 78, 250, 1903. t See Reichert, "University Medical Magazine," 5, 406, 1894; also Kemp, "Therapeutical Gazette," 1889, pp. 86 and 155. PHYSIOLOGICAL OXIDATIONS. 833 produced has failed to convince the majority of physiologists. The existence of a special set of heat nerves, in fact, is still unproved. Most physiologists, perhaps, believe that variations in heat pro- duction occur, as stated above, by alterations in the intensity of the oxidations in the muscles brought about by reflex excitation through the motor nerve fibers, and that a special set of heat fibers does not exist. We may at present adopt the conservative view that heat production and heat dissipation in the body are controlled not by a special heat-regulating apparatus composed of heat centers and heat nerves, but by the co-ordinated activity of a number of different centers in addition to the voluntary means already specified. The unconscious regulation of the body temperature is effected chiefly through the following centers : f 1. The sweat centers and sweat nerves. „ , ,. . ,. ) 2. The vasoconstrictor center and the vasoconstrictor Heat dissipation < nerye fibers tQ the gkin v. 3. The respiratory center. f 1. The motor nerve centers and the motor nerve fibers „ , ' +• ) to the skeletal muscles. Meat production i % The quantity and character of the food as deter- v mined by the appetite. Theories of Physiological Oxidations. — Lavoisier compared the oxidations in the body to the oxidation of organic substances in combustions at high temperatures. He supposed that the mo- lecular oxygen unites directly with the substances oxidized in one case as in the other. It soon became evident, however, that this direct analogy is not applicable. The material that is oxidized in the body — fats, carbohydrates, proteids — is consumed with a certain rapidity, — in the case of muscular contractions with great rapidity, — and we know that these same materials out of the body at a temperature of 39° C. are oxidized with extreme slowness. It became customary, therefore, to speak of the oxidations in the body as indirect, meaning thereby that the material is not acted upon directly by the molecular oxygen. Within recent years it has been shown that the oxidation in ordinary combustions — the burning of gaseous hydrogen, for instance — is not explained by assuming that the oxygen unites directly with the hydrogen. It is stated, for instance, that this combustion does not take place if both gases are entirely free from water vapor; the presence of water is necessary for the oxidation. Chemists are not agreed as to the exact nature of simple combustion, and it is therefore 'increasingly difficult to compare these processes with the oxidations in the body. Leaving aside the details of the process, it may still be believed that the metabolism of material in the body by means of which its heat energy is produced is at bottom comparable to ordinary combus- tions. Oxvgen is absolutely necessary to the process in each case; 53 834 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. the same end-products are formed and the same amount of heat is liberated in the one case as in the other. The fundamental point that the physiologist is attempting to solve is the means by which the body accomplishes these oxidations at such a low temperature. The theories suggested to explain this fact have changed naturally with the advance of chemical knowledge. After the discovery of ozone (Schonbein, 1840) and its great power of oxidation as com- pared with oxygen it was suggested that in some way the oxygen in the body is ozonized and is thus able to burn the food material. Gorup-Besanez showed that some of the oxidations that take place in the body can be successfully accomplished outside the body with the aid of ozone, especially in the presence of alkalies or alka- line carbonates. Others suggested that the oxygen in the body be- comes converted to atomic oxygen and is thus enabled to attack the tissue materials. Hoppe-Seyler formulated a theory according to which the living molecule is first split into smaller molecules by the hydrolytic action of ferments. In this process, as in fermentation, to which he compared it, hydrogen is liberated in the nascent or atomic state, and this hydrogen acting upon the oxygen forms water with the liberation of some atomic oxygen, which in turn oxidizes the split products of the fermentation. Others still (Traube) laid stress upon the possibility of the formation of hydrogen peroxid or similar peroxids which are then capable of effecting the oxidation of the body material. This latter theory, in modified form, still prevails.* The great amount of experimental and theoretical work upon the nature and cause of physiological oxidations has established pretty clearly two general beliefs which it is important to keep in mind. It has been shown, in the first place, that the amount of the oxidation is governed by the tissue itself and not by the quantity of oxygen present. The view that by increasing the amount of oxygen offered to the tissue the intensity of the oxidations can likewise be increased was formerly held and is still met with. It is often supposed, for example, that by breathing pure oxygen the oxidations of the body may be augmented. On the contrary, the facts indicate that when a sufficient supply of oxygen is provided any further increase has no immediate effect in aiding or hastening the oxidations. The intensity of the process is conditioned by the tissue itself. The initial stimulus or substance that sets going the whole reaction arises within the tissues. The second generalization that seems to be accepted more and more of recent years is that the oxidations of the body, those reactions that give rise to much heat, do not affect the living tissue itself. They take place under the * See Engler and Weissberg, " Kritische Studien iiber die Vorgange der Autoxydation, " 1904. PHYSIOLOGICAL OXIDATIONS. 835 influence of the living matter, or by the aid of substances (enzymes) formed by the living matter, but the material actually burnt is not organized living substance. As the living yeast cells break down sugar in the liquid surrounding them, so the living tissue cells metab- olize and oxidize the dead food material contained in the lymph and tissue liquid in which they are bathed. The opposite point of view was ably advocated by Pfliiger. This observer, in fact, ex- plained the mystery of physiological oxidations by assuming that the oxygen together with the food material is synthesized into the highly complex and unstable living molecules. The active intra- molecular movement within these molecules leads constantly to a breaking down, a splitting off of simpler molecules which consti- tute the products of physiological oxidation. The instability of the molecule is due to its size and the activity of the intramolecular movements, or, as Pfliiger expressed it, "The intramolecular heat of the cell is its life." This point of view, however, has not found acceptance of late years. It is implied or stated by most recent authors that the food material is attacked and oxidized outside the living molecule, in the form of fat, sugar, proteid, etc. The ten- dency for many years has been to show that these processes in the body are chemical changes that do not differ fundamentally from similar processes outside the body. The point . of view actually adopted by most workers is that the living matter effects its won- derful changes in the food material with the aid of intracellular ferments or enzymes. That such enzymes are formed, one may say generally in the tissues of the body, has been brought out in the pre- ceding chapters upon Digestion and Nutrition. It is necessary only to recall the facts that lipase, the fat-splitting enzyme, has been iso- lated from many tissues, and that in the liver and muscles and prob- ably other tissues there exist enzymes capable of converting glycogen to sugar or the reverse, and of destroying the sugar completely by the serial action of several intracellular enzymes. Finally, with regard to the proteid material it is now recognized that proteolytic enzymes are formed within many, if not all, of the living tissues. This point is demonstrated by the fact of autolysis, — that is, if living tissue is taken from the body, with precautions against contamination by bacteria, and while under perfect aseptic conditions is kept warm and moist, it will digest itself. The proteid is split up into the same simple hydrolytic products as are obtained by boiling it with acids. It has been shown that this digestion is due to enzymes — autolytic enzymes — formed within the living tissue. There is no doubt, therefore, of the existence of intracellular enzymes, and that these substances play a conspicuous part in the metabolism of food material. The lipase, the diastase, and the autolytic enzymes just referred to all belong to the group that cause hydrolytic cleavages, — 836 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. that is, they induce splitting or decomposition of the material by the introduction of water. The supposition has naturally been made that probably the oxidations of the body are effected also by enzymes which in some way activate the oxygen. Enz}rmes of this character have been found; they are designated in general as oxi- dases or as oxidases and peroxidases, the former term referring to those enzymes that effect oxidations in the presence of oxygen, while the latter is applied to certain enzymes supposed to act only in the presence of hydrogen peroxid. The most conspicuous of the oxidases found in the animal body is the one capable of oxidiz- ing aldehydes (salicylic or benzoic aldehyde) into the corresponding acids, and hence designated specifically as aldehydase. This enzyme has been extracted especially from the liver, lung, and spleen. An oxidase known as tyrosinase, first found in plant juice, has also been isolated from animal tissues. It oxidizes ty rosin with the formation of homogentisinic acid. So also oxidases are described capable of oxidizing xanthin to hypoxanthin or to uric acid.* The process of destructive metabolism of sugar in the body, glycolysis, may be effected, it will be remembered, by the tissue juices squeezed from the organs, or even by extracts of the tissues of muscle, liver, etc. It may well be believed, therefore, that the oxidation of this most important food material is accomplished by the action of one or more enzymes. Such facts as these lend great probability to the belief that eventually it will be shown that the oxidations in the body are effected by the influence of oxidases or peroxidases act- ing singly or in combination or in sequence with the hydrolytic enzymes. As a matter of fact, however, such an hypothesis is not by any means demonstrated. Oxidases capable of destroying proteid, fat, and carbohydrate material have not been actually iso- lated, but the idea seems to be a fruitful one. While it is perfectly obvious that more facts are needed before positive statements are warranted regarding the chemistry of the oxidations in the body, the view entertained regarding the general process in the body is that the material — proteid, fat, or carbohy- drate— is first split into simpler products by the action of a hy- drolytic enzyme, or a series of hydrolytic enzymes, formed in the cells. These reactions are not attended by any marked formation of heat. The split products thus produced are then acted upon by oxidases with the formation of carbon dioxid, water, etc., and the liberation of heat. A specific instance of this serial action has been given in reference to the oxidation of sugar (p. 794) : According to Stoklasa, the sugar is first split into lactic acid, and this into carbon dioxid and alcohol; the alcohol then by the action of a series of * For further details see Oppenheimer, " Die Fermente und ihre Wir- kungen, " second edition, 1903. PHYSIOLOGICAL OXIDATIONS. 837 oxidases is oxidized to acetic acid, formic acid, carbon dioxid, and water. From our present standpoint, the production of heat in the body, it is important also to bear in mind the general view advo- cated by Speck and others, — namely, that the chemical changes or metabolism of the body may be divided into two general classes : first, the heat-producing metabolism which results finally in the oxidation of the great mass of the food material and which is essential for the production of body heat, and, second, the tissue metabolism proper, — that is, the synthesis and disassimilation of the living substance itself. This latter metabolism varies prob- ably in the different tissues; it is concerned with the building up and breaking down of the living machinery and may be attended by the absorption as well as the liberation of heat, and the energy necessary for effecting these reactions is obtained from the heat energy liberated by the oxidation processes. In this last thought there is contained a suggestion which may serve as an explanation of the fundamental value of the physiological oxidations to the body. It may be supposed that these oxidations furnish the energy neces- sary for the nutritive metabolism of living matter. In those or- ganisms or cells that lead an anaerobic existence — that is, an exis- tence in the absence of free oxygen — the energy necessary for the process is obtained perhaps by hydrolytic changes alone. SECTION IX. THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. With the exception of the phenomenon of consciousness, no fact of life excites more interest and seems to offer greater diffi- culties to an adequate explanation than the function of reproduc- tion. The male cell, spermatozoon, and the female cell, ovum, unite to form a new cell which thereupon begins to grow rapidly and produces an organism that in all of its manifold peculiarities of structure and function is essentially a replica of its parents. The fundamental problems presented in this act of reproduction are those of fertilization and heredity. In the former we must ascertain why the union of the two cells is necessary or advanta- geous, and the secret of the stimulating influence upon growth that arises from this union. Under the term heredity we express the obvi- ous, yet mysterious, fact that the fertilized ovum of each species de- velops into a structure like that of its parents. Both of these im- portant problems are essentially of a physiological character, — that is, they deal with properties of the living material composing the reproductive cells; but, at present, biological investigation along these lines is largely in the morphological stage. The part of the sub- ject that can be studied with most success is the structural changes that are associated with fertilization and reproduction. Great, indeed wonderful, progress has been made during the last century, but it is needless perhaps to say that much remains unexplained, and that in this, as in so many other problems of nature, the greater our knowledge the clearer becomes our vision of the difficulties and complexities of a final scientific explanation. Outside these funda- mental problems there are other accessory functions connected, for instance, with the external genital organs which in a measure are of more immediate practical interest. In one way or another these functions are necessary or helpful to the final union of the repro- ductive cells. They form a part of the reproductive life which comes more immediately under our observation and control, and consti- tute, therefore, a subject which has been more accessible to in- vestigation. In the brief treatment given in the following chap- ters more emphasis is laid upon this side, the accessory phenomena of reproduction, than upon the deeper, more fundamental prob- 838 THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 839 lems, in view of the fact that the accessory phenomena are the ones which have at present the greater practical interest. The function of reproduction is often omitted from physiolog- ical courses, and the reason perhaps is partly that the structural features and the development of the embryo have been assigned to the department of anatomy, and partly because it is a function not essential to the maintenance of the existence and reactions of the organism. The reproductive organs might be eliminated en- tirely and the power of the body as an organism to maintain its individual existence not be seriously interfered with. The physio- logical importance of the reproductive organs lies not in their co-operation in the communal life of the various parts of the body, but in their adaptation to produce another similar being. We may explain, therefore, the co-ordinating mechanisms of the body without reference to the reproductive tissues, except so far as their supposed internal secretions affect general or specific metabolism. CHAPTER LII. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. The Graafian Follicle and the Corpus Luteum. — The functional value of the ovary is connected with the formation and rupture of the Graafian follicles, whereby an ovum is liberated. The pri- mordial follicles consist of an ovum surrounded by a layer of fol- licular epithelium. Beginning at a certain time after birth and continuing throughout the period of active sexual life, some of these primordial follicles develop into mature Graafian follicles and mi- grate to the surface of the ovary. The change consists in a pro- liferation of the follicular epithelium and the formation of a serous liquid, the liquor folliculi, between the layers of this epithelium. In the matured follicle there is a connective tissue covering, the theca folliculi, formed from the stroma of the ovary and consisting of two coats or tunics — the external and the internal. The cells in the internal tunic develop a yellowish pigment as the follicle grows, and are sometimes designated as lutein cells. Within the capsule formed by the internal tunic there is a layer of follicular cells known as the membrdna granulosa and attached to one side is a mass of the same cells, the discus proligerus — within which the ovum is imbedded. The follicular liquid lies between. This liquid increases in amount, and when the follicle has reached the JBRARV. J iff: 840 , n\VtJ*E PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. ■<^ ?) ;;)1V^>^ surtaceTtforms a vesicle projecting to the exterior. This projecting portion is nearly bloodless and thinner than the rest of the wall of the follicle. It is designated as the stigma. When fully mature the follicle ruptures at the stigma and the egg, together with the sur- rounding follicular cells of the discus proligerus and a portion of the membrana granulosa, is extruded, the egg being received into the open end of the Fallopian tube. According to Clark,* the rupture of the follicle is brought about by an increasing vascular congestion of the ovary. The tension within the ovary is thereby increased, the follicle is forced to the surface, and the circulation at the most projecting portion is interfered with to such an extent as to cause necrotic changes at the stigma, at which rupture finally occurs. After the bursting of the follicle its walls collapse, and the central cavity receives also some blood from the ruptured vessels of the theca. Later on the vesicle becomes filled with cells containing a yellow pigment. These cells increase rapidly and form a festooned border of increasing thickness around the central blood clot. The vesicle at this stage, on account of the yellow color of the new cells, is known as a corpus luteum. The structure thus formed increases in size for a period and then undergoes retrogressive changes and is finally completely absorbed. The duration of the period of growth and retrogression varies according as the egg liberated becomes fertilized or not. If fertilization does not occur, as is the case in the usual monthly periods, the corpus luteum reaches its maximum size within two to three weeks and then begins to be absorbed. It is frequently designated under these circumstances as the false corpus luteum (corpus luteum spurium) or corpus luteum of men- struation. In case the egg is fertilized and the woman becomes pregnant the life history of the corpus luteum is much prolonged. Instead of undergoing absorption after the third week it continues to increase in size by multiplication of the lutein cells during the first few months of pregnancy, and does not show retrogressive changes until the sixth month or later. The total size of the corpus in such cases is much larger than in menstruation, and it was des- ignated, therefore, by the older writers as the true corpus luteum (corpus luteum verum) or corpus luteum of pregnancy. Later observers agree that there is no essential difference in structure between the true and the false corpus luteum, although the former has a longer history and attains a greater size. The point of greatest structural interest in the corpus luteum is the origin of the yellow (lutein) cells. Histologists have been and still are divided upon this point; some believe that they arise from the cells of the membrana granulosa, others that they come from the connective tissue cells in the internal capsule (theca interna) of the follicle. * Clark, "Johns Hopkins Hospital Reports," 7, 181, 1898. D N \ THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGAZ&Js/' 841 The majority of writers seem to favor the latter view, the physiological importance of the corpus opinions also differ. Some regard it as simply a protective mechanism by means of which the empty space in the follicle is filled up by a tissue which is afterward easily absorbed, instead of by scar tissue. Others, how- ever, attribute to the lutein cells secretory functions of the most important character in connection with the subsequent develop- ment of the egg and the activities of the uterus. Some reference will be made to these views farther on. Menstruation. — The attainment of sexual maturity or puberty is marked by a number of visible changes in the body, but in the female the characteristic change is the appearance of the men- strual flow from the uterus. The age at which this phenomenon occurs shows many individual variations, but the average for temperate climates is given usually at 14 to 15 years. In the warmer countries the age is earlier, — 8 to 10 years, — and in the cold regions somewhat later, — 16 years. The racial characteristic in this respect is said to be maintained, however, after generations of residence in countries of a different climate, as is illustrated by the relatively early appearance of menstruation among Jews even in the colder countries. After the phenomenon appears it occurs at regular intervals of 28 days, more or less, and hence is known as the monthly period, menses, menstruation, or catamenia. The interval is not absolutely regular, and shows many individual variations within limits which may be placed at 20 to 35 days. Absence of the menstrual flow is designated as a condition of amen- orrhea. Certain premonitory symptoms usually precede the appearance of the menses, such as pains in the back or head or a general feeling of discomfort, although in some cases these symp- toms are absent. When these premonitory symptoms are unusually painful or serious and the flow is difficult or irregular the condition is designated as dysmenorrhea. The flow begins with a discharge of mucus, which later becomes mixed with blood. The quantity of blood lost is subject to individual variations, but it may amount to as much as 100 to 200 gms. The flow continues for 3 or 4 days and then subsides. Under normal conditions this phenomenon occurs regularly throughout sexual life, — that is, during the period in which conception is possible. If fertilization occurs the flow ceases normally during pregnancy and the period of lactation. At the forty-fifth to the fiftieth year the flow disappears permanently, and this change marks what is known as the natural menopause, climacteric, or change of life. The change is sometimes abrupt, sometimes very gradual, being preceded by irregularities in menstruation, and it is not infrequently associated with psychical and physical disturbances of a serious character. If at any time 842 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. during sexual life the ovaries are completely removed by surgical operation menstruation is brought to a close, this condition being designated as the artificial menopause. Structural Changes in the Uterus During Menstruation. — Men- struation is a phenomenon of the uterus. The lining mucous mem- brane, the endometrium, in the period of four or five days preceding the flow, becomes rapidly thicker and its superficial layers are con- gested with blood, and indeed in places small collections of blood may be noticed. Opinions differ very much as to the change under- gone by this thickened membrane during the flow. According to some authors, most of the membrane is thrown off and the blood escapes from the denuded surface mixed with pieces of the mem- brane. According to others, no material destruction of the mem- brane occurs, the blood that escapes being due to small capillary extravasations or perhaps mainly to a process of diapedesis. It would seem that the amount of destruction of the endometrium must be subject to individual variations. After the cessation of the flow the mucous membrane is rapidly repaired by regenerative changes in the tissues; the surface epithelium, if denuded, is re- placed by proliferation of the cells lining the uterine glands and the thickened, edematous condition of the membrane rapidly sub- sides during a period of six or seven days. While the escape of blood takes place only from the surface of the uterus, the other reproductive organs — the ovary, the Fallopian tubes, and even the external genital organs — share to some extent in the vascular con- gestion exhibited by the uterus during the period preceding' the menstrual flow. The mucous membrane of the uterus may be said to exhibit a constantly recurring menstrual cycle which falls into four periods: (1) Period of growth in the few (5) days preceding menstruation, characterized by a rapid increase in the stroma, blood-vessels, epithelium, etc., of the membrane. (2) The men- struation or period of degeneration (4 days), during which the capillary hemorrhage takes place and the epithelium suffers de- generative changes and is cast off more or less. (3) The period of regeneration (7 days), during which the mucous membrane returns to its normal size. (4) The period of rest (12 days), during which the endometrium remains in a quiescent condition. The Phenomenon of Heat in Lower Mammals. — The phenom- enon known as heat in lower mammals resembles, in many essential respects, menstruation in human beings, and they may be regarded as homologous functions. Heat is a period of sexual excitement which occurs one or more times during the year and during which the female will take the male. This condition lasts, as a rule, for several days, and in the female is accompanied by changes which recall those of menstruation. The external genital organs become THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 843 swollen and in many animals there is a discharge of mucus or mucus and blood from the uterus. Histologically the mucous mem- brane of the uterus undergoes changes similar to those of menstrua- tion,— that is, the membrane increases in size and becomes con- gested with blood, — and it exhibits a phase of degeneration during which some of the epithelial lining may be cast off and some hemorrhage occur. If sexual union is prevented during this period heat passes away in a few days, but recurs again at intervals which vary in the different mammals: 4 weeks in the monkey, mare, etc.; 3 to 4 weeks in the cow; 2£ to 4 weeks in the sheep; 9 to 18 days in the sow; 12 to 16 weeks in the bitch, etc. The re- currence of the period under these circumstances suggests at once the essential resemblance to the monthly periods of women. Ac- cording to Heape's most interesting observations upon monkeys (Semnopithecus),* some of these animals show a regular monthly flow lasting for 4 days except when conception takes place. The changes during heat must be considered as physiologically ho- mologous to those of menstruation. The sexual excitement that attends the condition in the lower animals is not distinctly repre- sented in man, although it is commonly said that in the period pre- ceding or following menstruation the sexual desire is stronger than at other times, but in the changes undergone by the uterus and the fact that these changes are connected, as a rule, with the liberation of an egg from the ovary (ovulation) the two phenomena are physiologically similar. Relation of the Ovaries to Menstruation. — It appears to be clearly demonstrated that the phenomenon of menstruation is de- pendent upon a periodical activity in the ovaries. When the ovaries are completely removed menstruation ceases (artificial menopause) and the uterus undergoes atrophy. When the ovaries are congenitally lacking or rudimentary, a condition of amenorrhea also exists. These facts and the connection of the ovaries with menstruation are further corroborated in a striking way by experi- ments upon transplantation or grafting of the ovary. This experi- ment has been performed upon lower animals (apes) as well as upon human beings. Removal of both ovaries in apes is followed by a ces- sation of menstruation. Transplantation of an ovary under the skin serves to maintain menstruation, but if subsequently removed this function disappears. f In the human being Morris and Glass ob- tained similar results. J: An ovary or a piece of an ovary trans- planted into the uterus itself or the broad ligament caused a re- * Heape, " Philosophical Transactions, Royal Society, " 185 (B), 1894, and 188 (B), 1897. t Halban, "Deutsche Gesellschaft f. Gynakol.," 9, 1901. t Glass, "Medical News," 523, 1899; Morris, "Medical Record," 83, 1901. 844 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. turn of the menstrual periods which had ceased after surgical re- moval of the glands, or brought on free menstruation in conditions of amenorrhea or dysmenorrhea. Many views have been proposed to explain this relationship between ovary and uterus. In most cases it has been assumed that the menstruation in the uterus is connected with the act of ovulation, — that is, the ripening and discharge of a Graafian follicle. Gynecologists, it is true, have accumulated facts to show that ovu- lation may occur independently of menstruation, but no one doubts that, as a rule, the two acts occur together, not simultaneously but in a definite sequence, and that the significance of menstruation is to be found in its physiological dependence upon the fate of the ovum. It was believed at first that the processes in the ovary influence the uterus by a nervous reflex. This view finds its most complete expression in the theory formulated by Pfliiger. According to this physiologist, the congestion of the uterus which leads to menstrua- tion and the congestion of the ovary which leads to ovulation are both reflex vasodilator effects due to the mechanical stimulation of the sensory nerves of the ovary by the growth in size of the fol- licle. As this structure develops the mechanical stimulus increases in intensity, the congestion in both organs becomes more pro- nounced and leads finally to the bursting of the follicle and the hemorrhage in the uterus. This very attractive theory does not, however, accord with the facts. Goltz and Rein* have shown by experiments upon dogs that when the nerves going to the uterus are completely severed from their central connections the animals can be fertilized, become pregnant, and give birth to a litter of young. Moreover, the experiments upon transplantation referred to above seem to show quite conclusively that a nervous connection is not essential to the influence that the ovary exerts upon the uterus. The present view, therefore, is that this influence is exerted through the blood, — the other great system connecting the organs with one another. The usual assumption is that the ovaries form an internal secretion which is given to the blood or lymph and upon reaching the uterine tissues serves to stimulate the mucous mem- brane to a more active growth. This theory has been elaborated most fully perhaps by Fraenkel,t who believes that this internal secretion is furnished by the yellow cells of the corpus luteum. This observer, from the results of operations upon women, believes that the ovum is normally discharged two weeks before menstrua- tion, and the resulting increased activity of the cells of the corpus * Rein, " Archiv f . die gesammte Physiologie, " vol. xxiii. f Fraenkel, ' Archiv f. Gynakologie," 68, 2, 1903. See also Ihm, " Monats- schrift f. Geburtshulfe u. Gynakol.," 21, 515, 1905, for discussion and exten- sive literature. THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 845 luteum furnishes the secretion which stimulates the uterus to the augmented growth that takes place in the premenstrual period. This view, as well as other most important functions which this author attributes to the specific tissue of the corpus luteum, have not been conclusively demonstrated. At present perhaps it is wiser to adopt the more cautious theory that some element in the ovary furnishes an internal secretion which is normally necessary to the nutrition of the uterus, and whose augmentation during the growth of a Graafian follicle leads to the greater metabolism charac- teristic of the premenstrual period. The Physiological Significance of Menstruation. — Naturally many views have been proposed to explain the significance of men- struation. According to the Mosaic law, it is a process of purifica- tion; others have seen in it a mechanism to remove an excess of nutriment in the body ; but since the period in which our knowl- edge of the structure of the organs concerned and of the histo- logical changes during the act became more definite, theories of the meaning of menstruation have usually assumed that it is a prepara- tion for the reception of the fertilized ovum. These views have taken two divergent forms according as the act of ovulation was believed to precede or to happen simultaneously with or subse- quently to the act of menstruation. According to one view, the swelling and congestion of the membrane constitute a prepara- tion for the reception of the fertilized ovum. If the ovum fails of fertilization, then degenerative changes ensue, and the membrane or a portion of it is cast off in the menstrual flow, while the re- mainder is absorbed. According to this view, menstruation is an indication that fertilization has not taken place.* This view is probably the one most generally accepted to-day, and falls in with the belief that ovulation normally precedes menstruation by a considerable interval. The other point of view was advocated especially by Pfliiger in connection with his theory of the common cause of ovulation and menstruation. He assumed that menstrua- tion occurs before the ovum reaches the uterus and that its physio- logical value lies in the fact that a raw surface is thus made upon which the ovum is grafted. Menstruation, according to him, is an operation of nature for the grafting of the fertilized ovum upon the maternal organism. This view finds some support in the fact that in some of the lower animals (dogs) the flow of blood precedes fertilization. The Effect of the Menstrual Cycle on Other Functions.— It is natural to suppose that such marked changes as occur in the * This view finds expression in the aphorisms : " Women menstruate because they do not conceive," Powers, and " The menstrual crisis is the physi- ological homologue of parturition," Jacobi. 846 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. ovary and uterus during the menstrual cycle should have an in- fluence upon other parts of the body. As a matter of fact, it is known that in general the sense of well-being varies with the phases of the cycle. At the time of or in the period just preceding the menstrual flow there is usually a more or less marked sense of ill- being or despondency, and a diminution in general efficiency. Among the various observations made by objective methods upon the functions of the different organs during these periods the most significant, probably, are those upon blood-pressure. According to Mosher,* the blood-pressure falls at the time of the menstrual periods. The curves obtained in these experiments are not entirely regular, but at or near the menstruation the blood-pressure falls slowly, the maximum fall being coincident with the appearance of the flow. It would seem probable that the fall of general blood- pressure is due directly to the vascular dilatation in the genital or- gans and in turn is responsible for some of the secondary phenomena observed in the organism as a whole. It is very suggestive to find that the author quoted above obtained similar periodical falls in blood-pressure in men, suggesting the idea that has frequently been expressed, that in man as well as woman there is a rhythmical activity of the genital organs, a reproductive cycle that in man may be referred to the development and extrusion of the sperma- tozoa in the testis, as in woman it is connected with the growth and expulsion of the ova in the follicles of the ovary. The Passage of the Ovum into the Uterus. — The means by which the ovum gains entrance to the Fallopian tubes has given rise to much speculation and some interesting experiments. It was formerly believed (Haller) that at the time of ovulation the fimbriated end of the Fallopian tube is brought against the ovary, the movement being due to a congestion or sort of erection of the fimbriae. This movement has not been observed, and, as experi- ments show that small objects introduced into the pelvic cavity are taken up by the tubes, it is believed that the cilia upon the fimbriae and in the tubes may suffice to set up a current that is sufficient to direct the movement of the ovum. Attention has been called to the fact that in animals with a bicornuate uterus the ova may be liberated from the ovary on one side, as shown by the presence of the corpora lutea, while the embryos are developed in the horn of the other side. As further evidence for the same possibility of migration it has been shown that the ovary may be excised on one side and the horn of the uterus on the other and yet the animal may become pregnant after sexual union. It would seem probable, therefore, that the -ovum is discharged into the pelvic cavity and may be caught up by the ciliary movements at the end of the tube * Mosher, "The Johns Hopkins Hospital Bulletin," 1901 THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 847 on the same side, or may traverse the pelvic cavity in the narrow spaces between the viscera and be received by the tube on the other side. Such a view explains the possible occurrence of true abdominal pregnancies, and suggests also the possibility that ova may at times fail to reach the uterus at all and may undergo de- struction and absorption in the abdominal cavity. In some of the lower animals — the dog, for example — provision is made for the more certain entrance of the ova into the tubes by the fact that the latter end in connection with a membranous sac of peri- toneum which envelopes the ovary. The sexual fertilization of the ovum is supposed to take place shortly after its entrance into the Fallopian tube, since spermatozoa have been found in this region, and the fertilized ovum, before reaching the seat of its im- plantation in the body of the uterus, has begun its development. By the act of coitus the spermatozoa are deposited at the mouth of the uterus, whence they make their way toward the tubes, being guided in their movements very probably by the oppos- ing force of the ciliary con- tractions in the uterus. It is known that the cilia of the tubes and uterus con- tract so as to drive inert objects toward the vagina and they carry the egg in this direction, but the spermatozoa, being moved by the contractions of their own cilia or tails, are stimulated to advance against this ciliary current. The act of ferti- lization of the ovum is preceded by certain preparatory changes in the ovum itself which are described under the term maturation. Maturation of the Ovum. — The process of maturation occurs before or just after the spermatozoon enters the ovum. At the time the latter is extruded from the follicle it is a single cell sur- rounded by a layer of follicular epithelium forming the corona ra- diata, which is subsequently lost. The egg proper consists of cytoplasm and a nucleus or germinal vesicle containing a nucleolus or germinal spot. Within the cytoplasm is a definite collection of food material or yolk which is sometimes designated as deutoplasm. The whole structure is surrounded by a membrane known as the zona radiata (Fig. 268) . Before or after the egg reaches the Fallopian Fig. 268. — Human ovum (Lee, modified from Nagel) : n, Nucleus (germinal vesicle) containing the ameboid nucleolus (germinal spot) ; d, deu- toplasmic zone; p, protoplasmic zone; z, zona radiata; s, perivitellin space. 848 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. tube its nucleus undergoes the changes preparatory to a mitotic division. These changes are essentially similar to those of ordi- nary cell division as represented schematically in Fig. 269. The nucleus at first presents the ordinary chromatin network, and in the cytoplasm lies the minute structure known as the centrosome. This latter divides into two daughter-centrosomes (6) which move to opposite sides of the nucleus and become surrounded by rays, each centrosome with its radiating system forming an astro- sphere. The chromatin material in the nucleus meanwhile has collected into larger threads known as chromosomes (c), and the nuclear membrane disappears (d). The number of chromo- somes is definite for each species of animal. The chromosomes arrange themselves equatorially between the astrospheres and then each divides longitudinally into two parts (/) . These parts migrate or are drawn toward their respective centrosomes (g, h, i), and this division is followed by a separation of the cytoplasm into two parts. Thus, two daughter-cells are formed, each containing the same number of chromosomes as the parent-cell, but only half the amount of chromatin material. The cell division results in a quantitative reduction of the chromatin material. In ordinary cell division the chromosomes again form a resting reticulum and a nuclear mem- brane and the chromatin substance increases in quantity. In the ovum a similar cell division takes place except that the daughter- cells are very unequal in size ; one is very minute and is known as the first polar body, the other as the ovum. After the formation and extrusion of the first polar body, the ovum again undergoes division into two unequal halves, giving rise to a second polar body. In this division, however, the chromosomes are divided transversely, and the ovum after the division is left with only half the number of chromosomes characteristic of the species. In the formation and extrusion of the two polar bodies the matured ovum has suffered both a quantitative and qualitative reduction in chromatin ma- terial, and is left with only half its number of chromosomes. Since the first polar body after its separation may again divide into two cells, the process of maturation results in the formation of four cells, three of which are polar bodies and may be regarded as abortive ova. The fourth, the matured ovum, retains practically all of the original cytoplasm, but has lost a part of its chromatin material and, according to Boveri, also its centrosome. The production of these four cells may be represented, therefore, by a schema of the kind shown in Fig. 270. The details of this process of forma- tion of the polar bodies and of reduction in chromatin material differ somewhat in different animals.* The process has not been * For details see Wilson, " The Cell in Development and Inheritance." Second edition, 1900, New York. x^3*^ io^ 2£^ 5.^ //. Fig. 269. — Schematic representation of the processes occurring during cell division. (Boveri.) THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 849 followed in the human ovum, but since it occurs in the eggs of all animals with sexual reproduction, so far as they have been studied, it is justifiable to assume that a similar change takes place in man. From a biological standpoint the reduction of chromosomes and the loss of the centrosome throw much light upon the necessity of fertilization by the male cell. The spermatozoon before it is ripe undergoes a process of maturation essentially similar to that des- cribed for the ovum. Two cell divisions take place with the for- mation of four spermatozoa, each of which, however, is a functional spermatozoon. In the process of division the number of chromo- somes in each cell is reduced by half. When the matured ovum and the matured spermatozoon fuse, therefore, each brings half the normal number of chromosomes, and the resulting fertilized ovum is a cell with its chromosomes restored to their usual number. The chromatin material is the essential part of the reproductive element. We have reasons to believe, in fact, that it is the sub- Ovarian egg. -First polar body. Mature egg WMW • • • Abortive ova resulting ^H|r from division of first ! polar body. Second polar body (abortive ovum). Fig. 270. — Schema to indicate the process of maturation of the ovum. — (Boveri.) stance which has the power of development and which conveys the hereditary structure specific to the animal. The process which causes each element to lose a part of this material before its union with the cell of the opposite sex is a provision by means of which the fertilized egg from which the offspring develops shall inherit the characteristics of each parent without increase in the typical amount of the chromosomes. The loss of the centro- some by the matured egg is interpreted by Boveri as follows : * This minute structure is the instrument by which the mechanical process of cell division is initiated and completed. Its loss by the matured ovum prevents this cell from further development, but in the act of fertilization the spermatozoon brings into the egg a new centrosome, or causes the formation of a new centrosome after its entrance, and thus immediately starts the process of cell mul- tiplication. From this standpoint the loss of the centrosome * For a popular presentation see Boveri, " Das Problem der Befruchtung. " Jena, 1902. 54 850 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. by the egg is a provision to necessitate sexual union, and thus insure the benefits that presumably are associated with the fusion of two cells originating from different individuals. Fertilization of the Ovum. — The spermatozoon comes into contact with the ovum probably at the beginning of the Fallopian tubes. The meeting of the two cells is possibly not simply a matter of accidental contact, although the number of spermatozoa dis- charged by the male at coitus is so great that there would seem to be little chance for the ovum to fail to meet some of them. Ex- periments upon the reproductive elements of plants indicate, how- ever, that the egg may contain substances which serve to attract the spermatozoon, within a certain radius, by that force which is described under the name of chemotaxis. However this may be, the egg unites with a spermatozoon and under normal conditions with only one. A number of the spermatozoa may penetrate the zona radiata, but so soon as one has come into contact with the cytoplasm of the egg a reaction ensues in the surface layer which makes it impervious to other spermatozoa. The spermato- zoon consists of three essential parts, — the head, the middle piece, and the tail. The last named is the organ of locomotion, and after the spermatozoon enters the egg this portion atrophies and disappears, probably by absorption. The head of the spermato- zoon represents the nucleus, and contains the valuable chromatin material. On entering the egg it moves toward the nucleus of the latter, meanwhile enlarging and taking on the character of a nu- cleus. The egg now contains two nuclei, — one belonging to it origi- nally, the female pronucleus; one brought into it by the sperma- tozoon, the male pronucleus. The two come together and fuse, — superficially at least, — forming the nucleus of the fertilized egg, or the segmentation nucleus. The middle piece of the spermatozoon also enters the egg, but its exact function and fate is still a matter of some uncertainty. Boveri believes that it brings into the egg a centrosome or material which induces the formation of a centro- some in the ovum and is therefore of the greatest importance in initiating the actual process of cell division which begins promptly after the fusion of the nuclei. In the segmentation nucleus the nor- mal number of chromosomes is restored, and it is believed that in the subsequent divisions of the cell to form the embryo the chromo- somes are so divided that each cell gets some maternal and some paternal chromosomes, and thus shares the hereditary characteris- tics of each parent. This view is represented in a schematic way by Fig. 271, taken from Boveri, the maternal and paternal chromo- somes being indicated by different colors. According to this descrip- tion, both egg and spermatozoon are incomplete cells before fusion. The egg contains chromatin for a nucleus, and a large cell body, Fig. 271. — Schematic representation of the processes occurring during the fertiliza- tion and subsequent segmentation of the ovum. — (Boveri.) The chromatin (chromo- somes) of the ovum is represented in blue, that of the spermatozoon in red. THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 851 cytoplasm, rich in nutritive material, but it lacks a centrosome, so that it can not multiply. The spermatozoon has also chromatin for a nucleus, and a centrosome or the material which may give rise to a centrosome, but it lacks cytoplasm, — that is, food material for growth. It would seem that if the spermatozoon could be given a quantity of cytoplasm it would proceed to develop an em- bryo without the aid of an ovum. This experiment has, in fact, been made by Boveri. Eggs of the sea-urchin were shaken violently so as to break them into fragments. If now a spermatozoon entered one of these fragments, which consisted only of cytoplasm, cell multiplication began and proceeded to the formation of a larva. On the other hand, it would seem to be equally evident that if a centrosome was present in the egg or some influence could be brought to bear upon it to make it form a centrosome it would develop without a spermatozoon. In some animals eggs do nor- mally develop at times without fertilization by a spermatozoon (parthenogenesis), the eggs that have this property probably pre- serving their centrosomes. Loeb* has shown, however, in some most interesting experiments that certain eggs, especially those of the sea-urchin (Strongylocentrotus purpura tus), which normally develop by fertilization with spermatozoa, may be made to de- velop by physicochemical means. His latest method is to treat the egg for a minute or two with an acid (acetic, formic, etc.), which causes the formation of a membrane. They are then placed for a certain interval in a hypertonic sea water, made by adding sodium chlorid to ordinary sea water. They are then transferred to normal sea water and after an hour or so they begin to multiply and eventually develop into normal larvae. Similar although less complete results were obtained previously by Morgan. Implantation of the Ovum. — After fertilization in the tube the ovum begins to segment and multiply, and meanwhile is carried toward the uterus, probably by the action of the cilia lining the tube. Upon reaching the cavity of the uterus it becomes attached to the mucous membrane, usually in the neighborhood of the fundus. The membrane of the uterus has become much thickened mean- while, and in this condition is known usually as the decidua. The portion to which the egg becomes attached is the decidua serotina, and it eventually develops into the placenta, the organ through which the maternal nutriment is supplied to the fetus. The ovum has made considerable progress in its development before reaching the uterus, having formed amnion and chorion, with chorionic villi. Some of the ectodermal cells in the chorion become specialized to form a group of trophoblastic cells which have a digestive action, * Loeb, "University of California Publications," 2, pp. 83, 89, and 113, 1905. See also Wilson, "Archiv f. entwick. Mechanik," 12, 1901. 852 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. and it is suggested that the activity of these cells enables the ovum to become attached to the decidual membrane and to hollow out spaces in which the chorionic papilla become inserted.* The further development of the egg into a fetus, the formation of the decidua graviditatis, and the placenta are anatomical features that need not be described here. Details of these structures will be found in works on anatomy, embryology, or obstetrics. The Nutrition of the Embryo — Physiology of the Placenta. — At the time of fertilization the ovum contains a small amount of nutriment in its cytoplasm. The amount, however, in the mam- malian ovum is small and suffices probably only for the initial stages of growth. When the ovum becomes implanted in the decidual membrane of the uterus the new material for growth must be ab- sorbed directly from the maternal blood of the uterus. Within a short time, however, the chorionic villi begin to burrow into the uterine membrane at the point of attachment, the decidua serotina, and the placenta gradually forms as a definite organ for the control of fetal nutrition. The details of histological structure of this organ must be obtained from anatomical sources. For the purposes of understanding its general functions it is sufficient to recall that the placenta consists essentially of vascular chorionic papillae from the fetus bathed in large blood-spaces in the decidual membrane of the mother. The fetal and the maternal blood do not come into actual contact; they are separated from each other by the walls of the fetal blood-vessels and the epithelial layers of the chorionic villi, but an active diffusion relation is set up between them. Nutritive material, proteid, fat, and carbohydrate, and oxygen pass from the maternal to the fetal blood, and the waste products of fetal metabolism — carbon dioxid, nitrogenous wastes, etc., pass from the fetal to the maternal blood. The nutrition of the fetal tissues is maintained, in fact, in much the same wray as though it were an actual part of the maternal organism. That material passes from the maternal to the fetal blood is a necessary inference from the growth of the fetus. The fact has also been demonstrated repeat- edly by direct experiment. Madder added to the food of the mother colors the bones of the embryo. Salts of various kinds, sugar, drugs, etc., injected into the maternal circulation may afterward be de- tected in the fetal blood. But we are far from having data that would justify us in supposing that the exchange between the two bloods is effected by the known physical processes of os- mosis, diffusion, and filtration. The difficulties in understanding the exchange in this case are the same as in the absorption of nour- ishment by the tissues generally. It is perhaps generally assumed that the chorionic villi play an active part in the process, func- * See Minot, " Transactions of the American Gynecological Society," 1904. THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 853 tioning, in fact, in much the same way as the intestinal villi. This assumption implies that the epithelial cells of the villi take an active part in the absorption of material by virtue of activities which can not be wholly explained, but which without doubt are due to the chemical and physical properties of the substance oi* which they are composed. This assumption does not mean that the simpler and better understood physical properties of diffusion and osmosis are not also important. The respiratory exchange of gases, the diffusion of water, salts, and sugar, may be largely controlled in this way. There are no facts at least which contradict such an assump- tion. The passage of fats and proteids, however, would seem to require some special activity in the chorionic tissue, which may be connected with the presence of special enzymes. Glycogen occurs in the placenta itself and in all the tissues of the embryo during the period of most active growth. In the later period of embryonic life, as the liver assumes its functions, the glycogen becomes more localized to this organ and disappears, except for traces, in the skin, lungs, and other tissues in which it was present at first in considerable quantities. It would appear, therefore, that glycogen (sugar) represents one of the important materials for the growth of the embryo, and that in the beginning at least the tissues generally have a glycogenetic power. The sugar brought to the placenta in the maternal blood passes over into the fetal blood and the excess beyond that immediately consumed is deposited in the tissues as glycogen. The body fat of the fetus is at first slight in amount, but after the sixth month begins to increase with some rapidity. The fat-forming tissues are in full activity, therefore, before birth, and function doubtless in the same way as in the adult. Before birth also the various organs begin to take on their normal activity. The kidney may form urine long before birth, as is shown by the presence of this secretion in the bladder, and, shortly before birth at least, it has the power of producing hippuric acid, as may be shown by injecting benzoates into the blood of the mother. The kidney functions of the embryo, how- ever, are doubtless performed chiefly by the placenta and the kidney of the mother up to the time of birth. That the liver also begins to assume its functions early is shown by the fact that from the fifth to the sixth month one may find bile in the gall-bladder. In the intestine, colon, there is found also a collection of excrement, the meconium, which shows that the motor and secretory functions of the intestinal canal may be present in the last months of fetal life. From the pancreas a proteolytic enzyme may be extracted at the time of birth or before, but the amylolytic enzyme is not formed apparently until some time later. It is stated, at least, that it is not present at birth. In general, it is evident that for a long period 854 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. the maternal organism digests and prepares the food for the embryo, excretes the wastes, regulates the conditions of temperature, etc., as it does for a portion of its own substance, but as the fetus ap- proaches term its tissues and organs begin to assume more of an independent activity, as indeed must be the case in preparation for the sudden change at birth. In this respect, as in all parts of the reproductive process, we meet with regulations whose mechanism is but dimly understood. Changes in the Maternal Organism during Pregnancy. — The two most distinct effects upon the mother that result from pregnancy are the growth of the uterus and of the mammary gland. The virgin uterus is small and firm, weighing from 30 to 40 gms., while at the end of pregnancy it may weigh as much as 1000 gms. This great increase in material is due partly to the growth of new muscular tissue and partly to an hypertrophy of the muscle already present. In the uterus at term the muscle cells are much longer and larger than in the organ before fertilization. The stimulus that initiates and controls this new growth is seemingly the fertil- ized ovum itself, but the physiological means employed are not comprehended. We know from experiments upon lower animals (Rein) that when all connections with the central nervous system are severed the fetus develops normally and the uterus increases correspondingly in size and weight. The influence of the ovum on the uterus must be exerted, therefore, either through some local nerve centers in the uterus, or, as seems much more probable, through some chemical stimulus which it gives to the organ. The effect of the presence and growth of the ovum on the mammary gland is treated in a separate paragraph below. In addition to these two visible effects it is evident that the growth of the fetus has an important influence on general metabolism and therefore upon the whole maternal organism. This fact is indicated by the marked changes often exhibited in the physical and mental con- dition of the mother. It is shown more precisely by a study of the nutritional changes. Numerous investigations have been made upon this side especially as regards the nitrogen equilibrium. Dur- ing the latter part of pregnancy especially the nitrogen balance is positive, — that is, nitrogen is stored as proteid, — due doubtless both to the growth of the embryo and the increase in material in the uterus and mammary gland. The proportion of ammonia in the urine increases during pregnancy and especially during labor (Slemmons*), — a result which may be due to some interference with the normal functions of the liver. Parturition. — The fetus "comes to term" usually in the tenth menstrual period after conception, — that is, about 280 days after * Slemmons, "The Johns Hopkins Hospital Reports," 12, 111, 1904. THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 855 the last menstruation. The actual time of delivery, however, shows considerable variation. Delivery occurs in consequence of contractions, more or less periodical, of the musculature of the uterus, and reflex as well as voluntary contractions of the abdom- inal muscles. It has been shown that delivery may occur when the nerves connecting the uterus with the central nervous system are severed, so that the act is essentially an independent function of the uterus, although under normal conditions the contractions of this organ are doubtless influenced by reflex effects through its extrinsic nerves. It has been shown that contractions of the gravid uterus may be caused by stimulation of various sensory nerves, and in women it is known that delivery may be precipitated prematurely by various mental or physical disturbances. The interesting prob- lem physiologically is to determine the normal factor or factors that bring on uterine contractions at term. Various more or less unsatis- factory theories have been proposed. Some authors attribute the act to a change in the maternal organism, such as mechani- cal distension of the uterus, a venous condition of the blood, a degenerative change in the placenta, etc., while others suppose that the initial stimulus comes from the fetus. In the latter case it is suggested that the increasing metabolism of the fetus is insuffi- ciently provided for by the placental exchange, and that therefore certain products are formed which serve to stimulate the uterus to contraction. The duration of the labor pains is variable, but usually they are longer in primiparse, ten to twenty hours or more, than in multip- ara?. After the fetus is delivered the contractions of the uterus continue until the placenta also is expelled as the " after-birth." During these latter contractions the fetal blood in the placenta is, for the most part, squeezed into the circulation of the new-born child. The hemorrhage from the walls of the uterus due to the rup- ture of the placenta may be profuse at first, but under normal con- ditions is soon controlled by the firm contraction of the uterine walls. The Mammary Glands. — At the time of puberty the mam- mary glands increase in size, but this growth is confined mainly to the connective tissue; the true glandular tissue remains rudi- mentary and functionless. At the time of conception the gland- ular tissue is in some way stimulated to growth. Secreting alveoli are formed, and during the latter part of pregnancy they produce an incomplete secretion, scanty in amount, known as colostrum. After delivery the gland evidently is again brought under the influence of special stimuli. It becomes rapidly enlarged and a more abundant secretion is formed. For the first day or two this secretion still has the characteristics of colostrum, but on the third or fourth day the true milk is formed and thereafter is 856 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. produced abundantly during the period of lactation under the in- fluence of the act of milking. If during this period a new con- ception occurs the milk secretion is altered in composition and finally ceases. On the other hand, if the act of nursing is aban- doned permanently the glands after a preliminary stage of turgid- ity undergo retrogressive changes that result in the cessation of secretory activity. The colostrum secretion that occurs during pregnancy and for a day or two after birth differs from milk in its composition and histological structure. It is a thin, yellowish liquid containing a larger percentage of albumin and globulin and a smaller percentage of milk-sugar and fat than normal milk. Under the microscope it shows, in addition to some fat droplets, certain large elements, — the colostrum corpuscles. These con- sist of spherical cells filled with fat droplets, and are most probably leucocytes filled with fat which they have ingested. Colostrum corpuscles may occur in milk whenever the secretion of the gland is interfered with, and their presence may be taken as an indi- cation of an incomplete secretion. The Connection Between the Uterus and the Mammary Gland. — The physiological connection between the uterus and the mammary gland is shown by the facts mentioned in the pre- ceding paragraph. That the ovary also shares in this influence either directly or through its effect on the uterus is shown by the fact that after complete ovariotomy the mammary gland under- goes atrophy. This undoubted influence of one organ upon the other might be exerted either through the central nervous system or by way of the circulation. There are indications that the secretion of the mammary glands is under the control, to some extent at least, of the central nervous system. For instance, in women during the period of lactation cases have been recorded in which the secretion was altered or perhaps entirely suppressed by strong emotions, by an epileptic attack, etc. This indication has not received satisfactory confirmation from the side of ex- perimental physiology. Eckhard found that section of the main nerve- trunk supplying the gland in goats, the external spermatic, caused no difference in the quantity or quality of the secretion. Rohrig obtained more positive results, inasmuch as he found that some of the branches of the external spermatic supply vasomotor fibers to the blood-vessels of the gland and influence the secretion of milk by controlling the local blood-flow in the gland. Section of the inferior branch of this nerve, for example, gave increased secretion, while stimulation caused diminished secretion, as in the case of the vasoconstrictor fibers to the kidney. These results have not been confirmed by others — in fact, they have been sub- THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 857 jected to adverse criticism — and they can not, therefore, be ac- cepted unhesitatingly. Mironow* reports a number of interesting experiments made upon goats. He found that artificial stimulation of sensory nerves causes a diminution in the amount of secretion, thus confirming the opinion, based upon observations upon the human being, that in some way the central nervous system exerts an influence on the mammary gland. When the mammary glands are com- pletely isolated from their connections with the central nervous system, stimulation of an afferent nerve no longer influences the secretion. Mironow states also that, although section of the ex- ternal spermatic on one side does not influence the secretion, sec- tion of this nerve on both sides is followed by a marked diminu- tion, and the same result is obtained when the gland on one side is completely isolated from all nervous connections. The dimi- nution of the secretion in these cases comes on very slowly, — after a number of days, — so that the effect can not be attributed to the removal of definite secretory fibers. Moreover, after ap- parently complete separation of the gland from all its extrinsic nerves, not only does the secretion, if it was previously present, continue to form, although less in quantity, but in operations of this kind upon pregnant animals the glands increase in size during pregnancy and become functional after the act of par- turition. This latter result confirms the older experiments of Goltz, Rein, and others, according to which section of all the nerves going to the uterus does not prevent the normal effect on lactation after delivery. Regarding the question of the existence of a se- cretory nerve, Baschf reports that extirpation of the celiac gan- glion or section of the spermatic nerve does not prevent the secretion, but causes the appearance of colostrum corpuscles. Experiments, therefore, as far as they have been carried, in- dicate that the gland is under the regulating control of the cen- tral nervous system, either through secretory or vasomotor fibers, but that it is essentially an automatic organ. The bond of con- nection between it and the uterus seems to be, in part if not entirely, through the blood rather than through the nervous system. It should be added that a definite connection between the nerve fibers and the epithelial cells of the gland has been described. { If this fact is corroborated it would amount to an histological proof of the existence of special secretory fibers, but the physio- logical evidence for the same fact is unsatisfactory. As was said in speaking of the histology of the gland, the se- * " Archives des sciences biologiques," St. Petersburg, 3, 353, 1894; t Basch, "Ergebnisse der Physiologie," vol. ii, part i, 1903. J Arnstein, " Anatomischer y\nzeiger," 10, 410, 1895. 858 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. creting alveoli are not fully formed until the first pregnancy. Dur- ing the period of gestation the epithelial cells multiply, the alveoli are formed, and after parturition secretion begins. As the liquid is formed it accumulates in the enlarged galactophorous ducts, and after the tension has reached a certain point further secretion is apparently inhibited. If the ducts are emptied, by the infant or otherwise, a new secretion begins. The emptying of the ducts, in fact, seems to constitute the normal physiological stimulus to the gland-cells, but how this act affects the secreting cells, whether reflexly or directly, is not known. Composition of the Milk. — The composition of milk is com- plex and variable. The important constituents are the fats, held in emulsion as minute oil droplets and consisting chiefly of olein and palmitin; casein, a nucleo-albumin which clots under the in- fluence of rennin; milk-albumin or lactalbumin, a proteid resem- bling serum-albumin ; lactose or milk-sugar ; lecithin, cholesterin, phosphocarnic acid, urea, creatin, citric acid, enzymes, and min- eral salts. It is well known also that many foreign substances — drugs, flavors, etc. — introduced with the food are secreted in the milk. An average composition is: proteids, 1 to 2 per cent.; fats, 3 to 4 per cent.; sugar, 6 to 7 per cent.; salts, 0.1 to 0.2 per cent. The fact that casein and milk-sugar do not exist preformed in the blood is an argument in favor of the view that they are formed by the secretory metabolism of the gland cells. The special com- position of the milk-fat and the histological appearance of the gland cells during secretion lead to the view that the fat is also constructed within the gland itself. Bunge has called attention to the fact that the inorganic salts of milk differ quantitatively from those in the blood-plasma and resemble closely the propor- tions found in the body of the young animal, thus indicating an adaptive secretion. This fact is illustrated in the following table giving the mineral constituents in 100 parts of ash: Young Pup. Dogs' Milk. Dogs' Serum. K20 8.5 10.7 2.4 Na20 8.2 6.1 52.1 CaO 35.8 34.4 2.1 MgO 1.6 1.5 0.5 Fe203....- °-34 014 012 P20- 39.8 37.5 5.9 CI 7.3 12.4 47.6 On account of the use of cows' milk in place of human milk in the nourishment of infants much attention has been given to the relative composition and properties of the two secretions. The chief difference between the two lies apparently in the casein. The casein of human milk is smaller in amount, curdles in looser THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 859 flocks than that of cows' milk, and seems to dissolve more easily and completely in gastric juice. The former also contains rela- tively more lecithin and less ash, particularly the lime salts. On the other hand, cows' milk contains less sugar and -fat. In using it, therefore, for the nutrition of infants it is customary to add water and sugar. The composition of cows' milk is so well known that it is easy to modify it for special cases according to the in- dications. The rules for this procedure will be found in works upon pediatrics. CHAPTER LIII. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE MALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. The sexual life of the male is. longer than that of the female. Puberty or sexual maturity begins somewhat later, — in tem- perate climates at about the fifteenth year; but there is no dis- tinct limitation of the reproductive powers in old age correspond- ing to the menopause of the female. At the time of puberty and for a short preceding period the boy grows more rapidly in stature and weight, and the assumption of its complete functions by the testis exerts a general influence upon the organism as a whole. One of the superficial changes at this period which is very evident is the alteration in pitch of the voice. Owing to the rapid growth of the larynx and the vocal cords the voice becomes markedly deeper, and the change is in some cases sufficiently sudden to cause the well-known phenomenon of the breaking of the voice. The neuromuscular control of the vocal cords becomes for a time un- certain. The completion of puberty can not be determined in the boy with the same exactness as in the girl, in whom menstruation furnishes a visible sign of sexual maturity. Much of the sexual mechanism may be functional long before the time of puberty, as is shown by the presence of sexual desire and the possibility of erection; but fully developed spermatozoa are not produced until this period, and indeed the presence of ripe and functional spermatozoa in the testis is the only certain sign that sexual ma- turity has been attained. Puberty consists in the maturation of the testis in the male, and of the ovary in the female. The Properties of the Spermatozoa. — The development and maturation of the spermatozoa in the testis has been followed successfully by histological means. The mother-cells of the sper- matozoa, the spermatocytes, give rise to four daughter-cells, sper- matids, each of which develops into a functional spermatozoon. The process in this case is something more than mere cell division, since in the spermatozoa eventually produced the number of chromosomes present in the nucleus — that is, the head of the sper- matozoon— are reduced by one-half. The process of production of the spermatozoa is therefore quite analogous to the maturation of the ovum during the formation of the polar bodies. The forma- tion and maturation of the spermatozoa may be represented by 860 THE MALE REPRODUCTIVE OR&tfJ^ 861 a schema similar to that used in the case of thech««^;s^a]$Dt^rn'> (Fig. 272). In the case of the ovum four ova are produce( only one is functional, and this one, the ripe egg, is characterized by its large amount of cytoplasm, its inability to undergo further cell division until fertilized, owing probably to its loss of the cen- trosome, — and the reduction of its chromosomes to half the num- ber characteristic of the body cells of the species. In the case of the spermatozoa, the four cells produced are all functional* and are characterized by the practical loss of cytoplasm, reduc- tion of chromosomes by one-half, and inability to multiply until cell material is furnished. The two cells supplement each other, therefore. Their union restores the normal number of chromo- somes, part of which are now maternal and part paternal; the egg supplies the cytoplasm and the spermatozoon the centrosome, — the two necessary factors in cell multiplication. The spermatozoa are produced in enormous numbers. It is calculated that at ejaculation each cubic centimeter of the liquid contains from sixty to seventy millions of these cells. The adult ripe spermatozoon is characterized as an independ- ent cell by its great motility, due to the cilia-like contrac- tions of its tail. Its power of movement or its vitality is retained under favorable con- ditions for very long periods. The most striking instance of this fact is found in the case of bats. In these animals copulation takes place in the fall and the uterus of the female retains the spermatozoa in activity until the period of ovulation in the following spring. Even in the human being it is believed that the spermatozoa may exist for many days in the uterus and Fallopian tubes of the female. In the semen that is ejaculated during coitus the spermatozoa are mixed with the secretions of the accessory reproductive glands, such as the seminal vesicles, the prostate gland, and Cowper's gland. The specific in- fluence of each of these secretions is not entirely understood, but experiments show that in some way they are essential to or aid greatly in maintaining the motility of the spermatozoa. Steinachf has found, for example, that removal of the prostate gland and * It is an interesting fact that in some cases (bees) two kinds of spermatids are formed by an unequal division of the spermatocyte, and the smaller of the two is abortive, as in the case of the polar bodies of the egg. t See Steinach, " Archivf. d. gesammte Physiologic," 56, 1894, and Walker, "Archiv'f. Anatomie u. Physiologic," 1899, p. 313. Primary spermatocyte. Secondary spermatocytes. Spermatids. — Spermatozoa. Fig. 272. — Schema to indicate the proc- ess of maturation of the spermatozoa. — (Boveri). 862 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. seminal vesicles in white rats prevents successful fertilization of the female, although the ability and desire to copulate is not interfered with. Direct experiments show that the secretion of the prostate gland maintains motility much more efficiently than a solution of physiological saline. It seems certain that the secretions of the mu- cous membrane of the uterus and Fallopian tubes exercise a similar favorable influence. Kolliker and others have investigated the action of many substances upon the motility of the spermatozoa, such as acids, alkalies, salts of various kinds and in different con- centrations, sugar, ethereal oils, etc. The union of spermatozoon and ovum is believed to take place usually in the Fallopian tube, and under normal conditions only one spermatozoon penetrates into the egg. The remainder of the infinite number that may be present eventually perish. The changes that take place during the process of fertilization have already been described (p. 850). Chemistry of the Spermatozoa. — Much chemical work has been done upon the composition of spermatozoa, particularly in the fishes. The results have been most interesting from a chem- ical standpoint, and biologically they are suggestive in that the analytical work has been done upon the heads of the spermatozoa. These heads consist entirely of nuclear material, and constitute the substance which conveys the hereditary characteristics of the father. Whatever progress may be made in the understanding of the chemistry of this material is a step toward the solution of the most difficult and mysterious side of reproduction, the power of hereditary transmission. Miescher in investigations upon the spermatozoa of salmon discovered that the heads are composed essentially of an organic combination of phosphoric acid, since designated as nucleic acid, united with a basic albuminous body, protamin. This view has been confirmed and extended by later observers, especially Kossel and his pupils.* The head of the spermatozoon, the male pronucleus in fertilization, might be de- fined, in the case of the fishes at least, as "a salt of an organic base and an organic acid, a protamin-nucleic acid compound. " The term protamin is used now to designate a group of closely related substances obtained from the spermatozoa of different animals. The special protamin of each species is designated ac- cording to the zoological name of that species; thus the protamin of salmon is salmin, of hering (Clupea harengus) clupein, and so on. The protamins are all strong bases; their aqueous solutions give an intense alkaline reaction, and they unite readily with various acids to form well-defined salts. They are albuminous bodies, giving the biuret reaction readily even without the addition of * For literature and details of the chemistry of spermatozoa see Burian, in " Ergebnisse der Physiologic," vol. iii, part i, 1904. THE MALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 863 alkali, and they are precipitated by most of the general precipitants of proteids, such as the neutral salts, the alkaloidal reagents, etc. Their solutions, however, are not coagulated by heat. The molec- ular formula for salmin is given as C30H57N17O6. When decom- posed by the action of acids they yield simpler basic products, the so-called hexon bases or diamido-acids, and particularly the base arginin (C6H14N402), which is contained in the protamin of the spermatozoa in greater abundance than in any other proteid. The protamins differ from most other proteid compounds by their relative simplicity; they contain no cystin grouping, therefore no sulphur; no carbohydrate grouping in most of the compounds examined; and no tyrosin complex. In the spermatozoa of some fishes the protamins are replaced by more complex compounds belonging to the group of histons which show properties somewhat intermediate between those of protamins and ordinary proteids. It should be added that these albuminous bases, protamins, have been obtained so far only from the spermatozoa of the fishes and some of the invertebrates. Efforts to obtain similar compounds from the sperm of mammals have been so far unsuccessful. The nucleic acid component of the spermatozoon resembles the same substance as obtained from the nuclei of other cells. In the spermatozoa of the salmon this nucleic acid has the formula C40H56- N14P4026. On decomposition by hydrolysis it yields at first some of the purin bases (adenin, guanin), and on deeper cleavage a number of compounds including the pyrimidin derivatives, thymin, uracil, and cytosin. . The Act of Erection. — In the sexual life of the male the act of erection of the penis during coitus offers a most striking physical phenomenon. During this act the penis becomes hard and erect, owing to an engorgement with blood. The structure of the corpora cavernosa and corpus spongiosum is adapted to this function, being composed of relatively large spaces inclosed in trabecular of connec- tive and plain muscle tissue, — the so-called erectile tissue. Many theories have been proposed to explain the mechanism of erection, but it is generally agreed that the work of Eckhard * demonstrated the essential facts in the process. This investigator discovered that in the dog stimulation of the nervi erigentes causes erection. These nerves are composed of autonomic fibers arising from the sacral por- tion of the spinal cord (see Figs. 104 and 105). They arise from the sacral spinal nerves, first to third (dog), on each side and help to form the pelvic plexus. They contain vasodilator fibers to the penis, as well as to the rectum and anus, and also visceromotor fibers to the descending colon, rectum, and anus. Eckhard, Loven, and others * Eckhard, " Beitrage zur Anatomie und Physiologie," 3, 123, 1863, and 4, 69, 1869. 864 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. have shown that when these fibers are stimulated there is a large dilatation of the arterioles in the erectile tissue of the penis and a greatly augmented blood-flow to the organs. If the erectile tissue is cut or the dorsal vein is opened the blood-flow under usual con- ditions is a slow stream ; but when the nervus erigens is stimulated the outflow is very greatly increased; according to Eckhard's measurements, eight to fifteen times more blood flows out of the organ. The act of erection is therefore due essentially to a vas- cular dilatation of the small arteries whereby the cavernous spaces become filled with blood under considerable pressure. The caver- nous tissues are distended to the limits permitted by their tough, fibrous wall. It seems probable that the turgidity of the organ is aided also by a partial occlusion of the venous outflow, which is effected by a compression of the dorsal vein by means of the muscle of Houston. This explanation of the act of erection, while no doubt correct so far as it goes, leaves undetermined the means by which the dilatation of the small arteries is produced. Vaso- dilator nerve fibers in general are assumed to produce a dilatation by inhibiting the peripheral tonicity of the arterial walls. If this explanation is applied to the case under consideration it forces us to believe that throughout life, except for the very occasional acts of erection, the arteries in the penis are kept in a constant condition of active tone. Moreover, on this view we should expect that section of the vasoconstrictor fibers to the penis by abolishing the tone of the arteries would also cause erection. These constrictor fibers arise from the second to fifth lumbar spinal nerves, and reach the organ by way of the pudic nerve or the hypogastric (pelvic) plexus. No such result of their section is reported and it seems that in the matter of erection the actual mechanism of the great dilata- tion caused by the nervi erigentes still remains to be investigated. The Reflex Apparatus of Erection and Ejaculation. — The dilatation of the arteries of the penis during erection is normally a reflex act, effected through a center in the lumbar cord. This center may be acted upon by impulses descending from the brain, as in the case of erotic sensations, or by afferent impulses arising in some part of the genital tract, — from the testes themselves, from the urethra or prostate gland, and especially from the glans penis. Mechanical stimulation of the glans leads to erection, and Eckhard showed in dogs* that section of the pudic nerve prevents this reflex from occurring, proving, therefore, that the sensory fibers concerned run in the pudic nerve. Stimulation of these latter fibers leads also to erotic sensations and eventually to the completion of the sexual orgasm. This latter act brings about the forcible ejection of the sperm through the urethra. It is initiated by contractions of the musculature of the vasa deferentia, ejaculatory duct, the seminal THE MALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 865 vesicles, and the prostate gland, which force the spermatozoa, to- gether with the secretions of the vesicles and prostate gland, into the urethra, whence they are expelled in the culminating stage of the orgasm by the rhythmical contractions of the ischiocavernosus and bulbocavernosus muscles, together with the constrictor urethra?. The immediate center for this complex reflex is assumed to lie in the lumbar cord, since, according to the experiments of Goltz, mechanical stimulation of the glans in dogs causes erection and seminal emission after the lumbar cord is severed from the rest of the central nervous system. Under ordinary conditions the act is accompanied by strong psychical reactions which indicate that the cortical region of the cerebrum is involved. It is interesting in this connection to find that electrical stimulation of a definite re- gion in the cortex* of dogs may cause erection and ejaculation. * Pussep, quoted from Hermann's " Jahresbericht der Physiologic,' ' vol. xi, 1903. 55 CHAPTER LIV. HEREDITY— DETERMINATION OF SEX— GROWTH AND SENESCENCE. Heredity. — The development of the fertilized ovum offers two general phenomena for consideration: First, the mere fact of mul- tiplication by which an infinite number of cells are produced by successive cell-divisions; second, the fact that these cells become differentiated in structure in an orderly and determinate way so as to form an organism of definite structure like those which gave origin to the ovum and the spermatozoon. In other words, the fertilized ovum possesses a property which, for want of a better term, we may designate as a form-building power. It is this last peculiarity which is included under the term heredity. The ovum develops true to its species, or, indeed, more or less strictly in accord- ance with the peculiarities of structure characteristic of its parents. The object of a complete theory of heredity is to ascertain the me- chanical causes — that is, the physicochemical properties — resi- dent in the fertilized ovum which impel it to follow in each case a definite line of development. The discussions upon this point have centered around two fundamentally different conceptions designated as evolution and epigenesis. Evolution and Epigenesis. — The earlier embryologists found a superficial explanation of this problem in the view that in the germ cells there exists a miniature animal already preformed, and that its development under the influence of fertilization consists in a process of growth by means of which the minute organism is unfolded, as it were. The process of development is a process of evolution of a pre-existing structure. Inasmuch as countless in- dividuals develop in successive generations, it was assumed also that in the germ cell there are included countless miniature organ- isms,— one incased, as it were, in the other. Some of the embry- ologists of that period conceived that the undeveloped embryos are contained in the ovum, — the ovists, — while others believed that they are present in the spermatozoon, the animalculists. Other embryologists pointed out that the fertilized egg shows no indication of a preformed structure, and therefore concluded that development starts from an essentially structureless cell and consists in the successive formation and addition of new parts which do not pre- 866 HEREDITY. 867 exist as such in the fertilized egg. This view in contradistinction to the evolution theory was designated as epigenesis. Microscopi- cal investigation has demonstrated beyond all doubt that the fer- tilized ovum is a simple cell devoid of any parts or organs resem- bling those of the adult, and the evolution theory in its crude form has been entirely disproved. Nevertheless the controversy be- tween the evolutionists and epigenesists still exists in modified form. For it is evident that in the fertilized ovum there may exist preformed mechanisms or complexes of molecules which, while in no way resembling anatomically the subsequently developed parts of the organism, nevertheless are the foundation stones, to use a figure of speech, upon which the character of the adult structure depends. Such a view in one form or another is probably held by most bi- ologists, since it avoids the well-nigh inconceivable difficulties of- fered by a completely epigenetic theory. If the fertilized ovum of one animal is in the beginning substantially similar to that of any other animal the epigenesist must ascertain what combination of conditions during the process of development causes the egg, in a dog, for instance, to develop always into a dog, and moreover into a certain species of dog resembling more or less exactly the parent organisms. The infinite difficulties offered to such a point of view are apparent at once. In this, as in other similar problems, experimental work is gradually accumulating facts which throw some light upon the matter and may eventually lead us to the right point of view. It has been made highly probable that the chro- matin material in the nuclei of the germ cells, the chromosomes, constitute the physical basis of hereditary transmission. In the fertilized egg, it will be remembered, half of the chromosomes come from the mother and half from the father, and there is good reason for believing that the maternal chromosomes are the bearers of the maternal characteristics, and the chromosomes derived from the spermatozoon convey the hereditary peculiarities of the father. Such a view, it will be noticed, implies at once preformed structures in the chromosomes and constitutes one form of an evolutionary hypothesis. This view is further supported by the interesting ex- periments of Wilson.* This author has shown that in certain molluscs (Dentalium or Patella) if a portion of the egg is cut off, the remaining portion upon fertilization develops into a defective animal that is not a whole embryo, but rather a piece or fragment of an embryo. Or if the fertilized egg after its first segmentation' is separated artificially into two independent cells each develops an embryo, but neither one is completely formed, — each is lacking in certain structures and * Wilson, "Science," February 24, 1905, for a popular discussion; also "Journal of Experimental Zoology," 1, 1 and 197, 1904, and 2, 371, 1905. 868 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. the two must be taken together to constitute an entirely normal animal. By experiments of this kind it has been shown that cer- tain definite portions of the egg are responsible for the formation of particular organs in the adult. If these portions of the egg are removed the organs in question are not developed. Facts of this kind lead to the evolutionary view that in the fertilized ovum there is a collection of different materials designated as formative stuffs each of which is specific, — that is, develops into a special structure. Many facts connected with the regeneration of parts, — regeneration of a lost leg in a crab, for example — may be used to support a similar view of the existence of specific formative stuffs in the cells of the body.* Wilson has suggested an attractive theory which seems to account for the facts known at present and forms an acceptable com- promise between the extremes of epigenesis and evolution. Accord- ing to him, the germ (fertilized ovum) contains two elements, one of which undergoes a development that is essentially epigenetic, while the other contains a preformed structure which controls and deter- mines the course of development. The first is represented by the cytoplasm of the egg, the second by the chromatin (chromosomes) of the nucleus. The latter have specific structures, and under their influence the nutritive undifferentiated material of the cytoplasm is modified to form specific formative stuffs differing in character in the developing ova of different animals. Many interesting gen- eral theories of heredity have been proposed by Darwin, Nageli, Weissmann, Mendel, Galton, Brooks, and others. It is impossible to give here an outline of these theories ; the reader is referred for such information to special treatises on the subject. f Determination of Sex. — The conditions which lead to the determination of the sex of the developing ovum have attracted much investigation and speculation. In the absence of precise data very numerous and oftentimes very peculiar theories have been advanced. J Such views as the following have been main- tained: that the sex is determined by the ova alone; that it is determined by the spermatozoa alone; that one side (right ovary or testis) contains male elements, the other female; that the sex is a result of the interaction of the ovum and spermatozoon, the most virile element producing its own sex, or according to another possibility "the superior parent produces the opposite sex"; that the sex depends on the time relation of coitus to menstruation, * For a discussion of these facts and for various hypotheses see Morgan, " Regeneration." New York, 1901. fHertwig, "The Biological Problems of To-day," and Delage, "L'heredite et les grands problemes de la biologie generate, " 1903. % For recent accounts of the various theories and discussion see Mor- gan, "Popular Science Monthly," December, 1903; Lenhossek, "Das Problem der geschlechtsbestimmenden Ursachen," 1903. DETERMINATION OF SEX. 869 fertilization before menstruation favoring male births, after men- struation female births; that it depends upon the nutritive con- ditions of the ovum during development or of the maternal parent ; that it depends upon the relative ages of the parents; that there are preformed male and female ova and male and female sper- matozoa, etc. What we may call the scientific study of the problem began with the collection of statistics of births. Statistics in Europe of 5,935,000 births indicate that 106 male children are born to 100 female, and the data from other countries show the same fact of an excess of male children. Examination of these statistics with reference to determining conditions led to the formulation of the so-called Hofacker-Sadler law or laws, which may be stated as follows: (1) When the man is older than the woman the ratio of male births is increased (113 to 100). (2) When the parents are of equal age the ratio of female births is increased (93.5 males to 100 females). (3) When the woman is older the ratio of female births is still further increased (88.2 to 100). These laws have been corroborated by some statisticians and contradicted or modi- fied by others. Ploss attempted to show that poor nutritive con- ditions affecting the parents, especially the mother, favor the birth of boys. Dusing combined these results in a sort of general compensatory law of nature, according to which a deficiency in either sex leads, by a process of natural selection, to an increase in the births of the opposite sex. Thus, when males are few in number, — as the result, for instance, of wars, — females marry later, and according to the Hofacker-Sadler law more males are produced. When males are in excess early marriages are the rule and this condition favors an excess of female births. However interesting these statistics may be, it is very evident that they do not touch the real problem of the cause of the determination of sex. Modern work has turned largely to observations and direct experiments upon the lower animals, particularly the inverte- brates, with the result that a very large number of facts have been collected of a most interesting kind, but difficult as yet to interpret so as to formulate a general law. The trend of modern work, however, tends to oppose an older view founded largely upon experiments on frogs, bees, and wasps, according to which the sex is not determined at or before fertilization, but is con- trolled or may be controlled by the conditions of nourishment during development, favorable conditions of nutriment leading to the development of female cells from the germinal epithelium of the embryo. The general opinion at present seems to be that the sex of the embryo is determined in the egg before fertilization or at the time of fertilization. This view assumes substantially 870 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. that there are male and female eggs to begin with, and that the determination of sex resides in the maternal organism alone. Some of the facts that support this view with more or less con- clusiveness are as follows: (1) In certain worms (Dinophilus) eggs of two sizes are produced; the large eggs on fertilization develop always into females, the small ones into males. Similar facts are recorded for other animals (Hydatina). (2) Many species of in- vertebrates exhibit the phenomenon of parthenogenesis, — that is, the eggs of the mother develop without fertilization. In some cases this method forms the only means of reproduction, and the individuals of the race are all females. But in other animals re- production is effected either by parthenogenesis or by fertilization according to the conditions, — change of seasons, etc. Among these latter animals it may be shown, in some cases at least, that the part henogene tic eggs may give rise either to males or females, — ■ a fact which accords with the hypothesis of the existence of male and female eggs in the mother. (3) In man twins may be born. These twins may be of two kinds. First, those that are developed from two different eggs, each of which has its own chorion and develops its own placenta. This kind may be designated as false twins, and in the matter of sex they may be male and female, or both male, or both female. The matter varies as in the statis- tics of births in general. In the other group, however, of true twins or identical twins, the two embryos are developed from a single ovum and are included in a single chorion. In such cases the sexes of the twins are always the same, they are both boys or both girls. This fact speaks strongly in favor of the view that the sex may be predetermined in the ovum, which may be either male or female. However, if we grant the fundamental fact, so for as the ova are concerned, that they are either male or female at the time of formation or are made so during the process of growth and maturation, it is still logically possible that there may also be male and female spermatozoa, and that in the union of the two cells the sex of the fertilized ovum may be referable either to the ovum or spermatozoon. It is not justifiable to assert that the paternal organism is entirely without influence upon the sex of the offspring. In fact, in the case of honey bees it is observed that if the egg of the queen bee is unfertilized it develops into a male, but, if fer- tilized, into a female, thus indicating a determining influence upon the part of the male element. If, on the basis of this fact, we assume the existence of male and female eggs and male and female spermatozoa the question of the sex of the offspring would seem to depend upon which of the sex-determining structures in the two cells predominates after union. Our problem still remains unsolved, although reduced to a narrower field for observation GROWTH AND SENESCENCE. 871 and experiment. It may be stated, in conclusion, that Morgan advocates a conservative view according to which the egg, as far as sex is concerned, is in a sort of balanced condition and may be thrown to the side of male development or female development according to the nature of the stimulus received. Fertilization by the spermatozoon presumably, on this view, is only one of a number of stimuli which may determine the direction of sex development. Growth and Senescence. — The body increases rapidly after birth in size and weight. It is the popular idea that the rate of growth increases up to maturity and then declines as old age ad- vances. As a matter of fact, careful examination of the facts shows that the rate of growth decreases from birth to old age, although not uniformly. At the pubertal period and at other times its downward tendency may be arrested for a time. But, speaking generally, the maximum rate of growth is reached some time during the intra- uterine period, and after birth the curve falls steadily. Senescence has begun to appear at the time we are born.* Thus, according to the statistics of Quetelet, the average male child weighs at birth 6J pounds. At the end of the first year it weighs 18J pounds, a gain of 12 pounds. At the end of the second year it weighs 23 pounds, a gain of only 4J pounds, and so on, the rate of increase falling rap- idly with advancing years. The actual statistics of growth have been collected and tabulated with great care by a number of ob- servers; for this country especially by Bowditch, Porter, and Beyer.f An interesting feature of the records collected by Bowditch is the proof that the prepubertal acceleration of growth comes earlier in girls than in boys, so that between the ages of twelve and fifteen the average girl is heavier and taller than the boy. Later, the boy's growth is accelerated and his stature and weight increase beyond that of the girl. It appears from the examinations made upon school children by Porter and by Beyer that a high degree of physical development is usually associated with a corresponding pre-eminence in mental ability. The signs of old age may be de- tected in other ways than by observations upon the rate of growth. Changes take place in the composition of the tissues; these changes, at first scarcely noticeable, become gradually more obvious as old age advances. The bones become more brittle from an increase in their inorganic salts, the cartilages become more rigid and calca- reous, the crystalline lens gradually loses its elasticity, the muscles lose their vigor, the hairs their pigment, the nuclei of the nerve * See Minot, "Journal of Physiology," 12, 97. f See Bowditch, " Report of State Board of Health of Massachusetts," 1877, 1879, and 1891; Porter, "Transactions, Academy of Science," St. Louis, 1893-94; Beyer, "Proceedings, United States Naval Institute," 21, 297, 1895. 872 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. cells become smaller, and so on. In every way there is increasing evidence, as the years grow, that the metabolism of the living mat- ter of the body becomes less and less perfect; the power of the protoplasm itself becomes more and more limited, and we may suppose would eventually fail, bringing about what might be called a natural death. As a matter of fact, death of the organism usually results from the failure of some one of its many complex mechanisms, while the majority of the tissues are still able to maintain their exis- tence if supplied with proper conditions of nourishment. The phys- iological evidences of an increasing senescence warrant the view, however, that death is a necessary result of the properties of living matter in all the tissues except possibly the reproductive elements. The course of metabolism is such that it is self-limited, and even if perfect conditions were supplied natural death would eventually result. We do not understand the nature of these limitations, — that is, the ultimate causes of senescence. Many examples of unusual longevity are on record, the most authentic being probably that of Thomas Parr. An account of his life and the results of a postmor- tem examination by Harvey are given in volume iii of the u Philo- sophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London." "He died after he had outlived nine princes, in the tenth year of the tenth of them, at the age of one hundred and fifty-two years and nine months." The immediate cause of his death was attributed to a change of food and air and habits of life, as he was brought from Shrop- shire to London, '* where he fed high and drunk plentifully of the best wines." With reference to the phenomenon of senescense as a neces- sary attribute of living matter, Weissmann has called attention to the fact that inasmuch as the species continues to exist after the in- dividual dies, we must believe that the protoplasm of the repro- ductive elements is not subject to natural death, but has a self- perpetuating metabolism which under proper conditions makes it immortal. Weissmann* designates the protoplasm of the germ cells as germ-plasm, that of the rest of the body as somatoplasm, and inasmuch as the former continues to propagate itself indefinitely under proper conditions, while the latter has a limited existence, he concludes that originally protoplasm possessed the property of potential immortality. That is, barring accidents, disease, etc., it was capable of reproducing itself indefinitely. He assumes, more- over, that this property is exhibited at present in many of the sim- pler forms of life, such as the ameba. This latter phase of his theory has been the subject of much interesting investigation,! but con- * Weissmann, " Essays upon Heredity and Kindred Biological Prob- lems"; also " Germ-plasm " in the " Contemporary Science Series." t See Maupas, " Archives de zoologie experimentale et generale," 6, 165, 1888; Joukowsky, "Inaugural Dissertation," Heidelberg, 1898; Gotte, " Ueber den Ursprung des Todes," 1883. GROWTH AND SENESCENCE. 873 elusive results have not been reached. Assuming that the poten- tial immortality exhibited by the reproductive cells was originally a general property of protoplasm, Weissman conceives that the phe- nomenon of senescence and death exhibited by other cells, somato- plasm, is a secondary property, which was acquired as a result of variation and was preserved by natural selection because it is an advantage in the propagation of the species. An actual immor- tality of the entire organism, — that is, the property of indefinite existence except as death might be caused by accidental occur- rences of various kinds — would be a disadvantage in many ways. The vast increase in the number of individuals 'might exceed the capacity of nature to provide for, the retention of the maimed and imperfect would make many useless mouths to feed, and perhaps the evolution of higher and more perfect forms by the slow action of variation and natural selection would be retarded. From this point of view senility and natural death constitute a beneficial adaptation, acquired because of its utility to the race, on the one hand, and, on the other, because, after the beginning of a differen- tiation in function among the cells, the possession of immortality by all the cells was no longer of any value to the race, and therefore was not brought under the preserving influence of natural selection. APPENDIX. PROTEIDS AND THEIR CLASSIFICATION. Definition and General Structure. — Proteids or albumins are complex organic compounds containing nitrogen which, although differing much in their composition, are related in their properties. They are formed by living matter, and occur in the tissues and liquids of plants and animals, of which they form the most characteristic constituent. On ultimate analy- sis they are all found to contain carbon, hydrogen, oxygen, and nitrogen; most of them contain also some sulphur, and some, in addition, phosphorus or iron. As usually obtained, they leave also some ash when incinerated, showing that they hold in combination some inorganic salts. Percentage analyses of the most common proteids of the body show that the above named constituents occur in the following proportions: Carbon 50 to 55 per cent. Hydrogen 6.5 to 7.3 " " Nitrogen 15 to 17.6 " " Oxygen 19 to 24 " Sulphur 0.3 to 2.4 " " The clearest insight into the structure of the proteid molecule has been obtained by a study of its decomposition products. When submitted to the action of proteolytic enzymes, or putrefaction, or acids at high temperatures, the large molecules split into a number of simpler bodies in consequence of hydrolytic cleavage. These end-products are very numerous, and, while they differ somewhat for the different proteids, yet a number of them are the same for all proteids. The great variety in the end-products is an in- dication of the complexity of the molecule, while their similarity is proof that the various proteids are all built, so to speak, upon a similar plan by the union of certain groupings which may be more numerous in one proteid than another. This fact becomes evident from a brief consideration of the prod- ucts obtained by hydrolytic cleavage with acids. The groupings represented by the following compounds may be supposed to exist preformed in proteid molecules, some possibly containing them all, some only a portion of the list, while the different groups vary in their proportional amounts in the various proteids: 1. Amido-acetic acid (glycocoll). 2. Amidopropionic acid (alanin). 3. Amidovalerianic acid. 4. Amidocaproic acid (leucin). 5. Amidosuccinic acid (aspartic acid). 6. Amidoglutaric acid (glutaminic acid). 7. Phenylamidopropionic acid (phenylalanin). 8. Oxyphenylamidopropionic acid (tyrosin). 9. Skatolamido-acetic acid (tryptophan). 10. Guanidinamidovalerianic acid (arginin). 11 Diamidocaproic acid (lysin). 12. Histidin (amidomethyl-dihydropyrimidin-carbonic acid). 13. Amido-oxypropionic acid (serin). 14. Amidothiolactic acid (cystein or cystin). 15. a-pyrrollidin-carbonic acid. 16. Oxy-a-pyrrollidin-carbonic acid. 874 PROTEIDS AND THEIR CLASSIFICATION. 875 These split products are all amido-acids, some of them belonging to the fatty acid (aliphatic) series of carbon compounds, some to the aromatic (carbocyclic) series, and some to the heterocyclic (pyrrol, indol) series. In what may be considered the simplest proteids occurring in nature, — namely, the protamins found in the spermatozoa, only from four to six of these groups occur, while in some of the more familiar proteids, such as serum-albumin or casein, a much larger number is found. The a-amido-acids of which these H end-products consist all contain the grouping — C — NH2, and Fischer has COOH shown that such bodies possess the property of combining with one another to make complex molecules containing two, three, or more groups of amido- acids. The combination takes place with the elimination of water formed by the union of the OH of the carboxyl (COOH) group in one acid and the H of the amido (NH2) group in another. Thus, two molecules of amido-acetic acid (glycocoll) may be made to unite to form a compound, glycylglycin, as follows : NH2CH2COOH + NH2CH2COOH — H20 = NH2CH2CONHCH2COOH. Glycocoll. Glycocoll. Glycylglycin. Compounds of this kind are designated by Fischer as peptids. When formed from the union of two amido-acids they are known as dipeptids, from three as tripeptids, etc. The more complicated compounds of this sort, the poly- peptids, begin to show reactions similar to those of the proteids. Some of them give the biuret reaction, some are acted upon and split by proteolytic enzymes. It seems justifiable, therefore, to consider proteids as essentially polypeptid compounds of greater or less complexity, — that is, they are acid- amids formed by the union of a number of a-amido-acid compounds. This conception of the structure of the proteid molecule explains a number of their general characteristics, — for instance: (1) The fact that they are all decomposed and yield similar products under the influence of proteolytic enzymes or boiling dilute acid. (2) The fact that the proteids are all so alike in their general properties in spite of the great differences in the com- plexity of their molecular structure. (3) The fact that they show both basic and acid characters. (4) The fact that they all give the biuret reac- tion* (see below). In addition to the amido-acids some proteids — egg-albumin, for example — yield a carbohydrate body upon decomposition. The carbohydrate ob- tained is an amido-sugar compound, usually glucosamin, C6H13N03. It is detected by its reducing action and by the formation of an osazone. It seems probable, therefore, that some of the proteids at least contain such a group- ing as part of the molecular complex, but at present it is undetermined how many possess this peculiarity of structure. General Reactions of the Proteids. — It is evident from what has been said in the preceding paragraph that proteids may give different reactions according to the kinds of groupings contained in the molecule. The reac- tions common to all proteids are few in number, the most certain perhaps being the biuret reaction, the hydrolysis by proteolytic enzymes or putre- factive organisms, and the nature of the split products formed by these latter hydrolyses or by the action of boiling dilute acids. A very large number of reactions, however, have been described which hold for some or all of the proteids usually found in the tissues and liquids of the body. These reactions may be described under two heads: (1) precipitation of the proteid when in solution; (2) color reactions. * For further details see Cohnheim, "Chemie der Eiweisskorper," second edition, 1904; or Hammarsten, "Physiological Chemistry," translated by Mandel, fourth edition, New York, 1904. 876 APPENDIX. I. Precipitants. — For one or another proteid the following reagents cause precipitation : 1. The addition of an excess of alcohol. 2. Boiling (heat coagulation). 3. The addition of mineral acids, — e. g., nitric acid. 4. The salts of the heavy metals, — e. g., acetate of lead, copper sul- phate, etc. 5. Addition of neutral salts of the alkalies to a greater or less degree of concentration, — e. g., sodium chlorid, ammonium sulphate. 6. Ferrocyanid of potassium after previous acidification by acetic acid. 7. Tannic acid after previous acidification by acetic acid. 8. Phosphotungstic or phosphomolybdic acid in the presence of free mineral acids. 9. Iodin in solution in potassium iodid, after previous acidification with a mineral acid. 10. Picric acid in solutions acidified by organic acids. 11. Trichloracetic acid. This list might be extended still further, but it comprises the precipi- tating reagents that are ordinarily used. Some of them, particularly Nos. 7, 8, and 9, give reactions in solutions containing excessively minute traces of proteid. 12. Precipitins. In this connection a brief reference may be made to the interesting group of bodies known as precipitins. As stated on p. 387, the animal organism has the power, when foreign cells are injected into it, of forming anti-bodies by a specific biological reaction. It has been discovered that anti-bodies, or as they are called in this case, precipitins, may be produced in the same way if proteid solutions or solutions of animal tissue are in- jected into the circulation. Thus, if cows' milk be injected under the skin of a rabbit there will be produced within the rabbit's blood a precipitin which is capable of precipitating the casein of cows' milk, although it may have no action on the milk of other animals. In the same way any given foreign proteid, when injected under the skin of an animal, may cause the production of a pre- cipitin capable of precipitating that particular proteid from its solutions. The precipitin is not absolutely specific for the proteid used to produce it, but nearly so. If a rabbit is immunized with human blood a precipitin is produced in the animal's blood which causes a precipitate when mixed with human blood or with that of some of the higher monkeys, but gives no reaction with the blood of other mammals. The reaction may be used, therefore, in a measure to test the blood-relationship of different animals.* It has been suggested that the reaction may also be of practical importance in medicolegal cases, in determining whether a given blood-stain is or is not human blood. For such a pur- pose a human antiserum is first produced by injecting human serum into a rabbit. The serum of the rabbit is then mixed with an extract of the suspected blood-stain made with salt solution; if a precipitate forms it proves that the blood stain is human blood provided the possibility of its being monkey's blood is excluded. Concerning the nature of the precipitins, little is known. They combine quantitatively with the proteid precipitated and they are inactivated (hematosera) by a temperature of 70° C. Their reactions are not sufficiently specific to be used as a means of de- tecting or distinguishing closely related proteids. 77. The Color Reactions of Proteids. 1. The biuret reaction. The proteid solution is made strongly alkaline with caustic soda or potash and a few drops of a dilute solution of * For many interesting experiments, and the literature see Nuttall, " Blood Immunity and Relationship." Cambridge, 1904. PROTEIDS AND THEIR CLASSIFICATION. 877 copper sulphate are added carefully so as to avoid an excess. A purple color is obtained. Some proteids (peptones) give a red purple, others a blue purple. If only a blue color, without any mixture of red, is obtained, no proteid is present. At present this reaction gives the best single test for proteid. It obtains CONH its name from the fact that it is given by biuret HN ^pomtj2? a compound that may be formed by heating urea. Two molecules of urea give off a molecule of ammonia and form biuret. 2. The Millon reaction. The proteid solution is boiled with Millon's reagent. The solution or the precipitate, if one is formed, takes on a reddish color, which varies in intensity with different proteids. Millon's reagent consists of a solution of mercuric nitrate in nitric acid containing some mercurous nitrate. This reaction is supposed to be given by the tyrosin grouping in the proteid molecule, and fails, therefore, with those proteids in which tyrosin is not present. 3. The xanthoproteic reaction. Nitric acid is added to strong acid reaction and the solution is then boiled. After cooling ammonia is added. The ammonia causes the development of a deep-yellow color if proteid is present. This reaction is supposed to be due to the presence in the molecule of the groupings belonging to the aromatic series. 4. Adamkiewicz's reaction: A mixture is made of one volume of con- centrated sulphuric and two volumes of glacial acetic acid; if the proteid solution is added to this mixture and warmed a reddish- violet color is obtained. According to Hopkins and Cole, the re- action depends upon the presence of glyoxylic acid in the acetic acid. This reaction seems to be due to the tryptophan grouping in the proteid molecule. 5. Liebermann's reaction. Dry proteid purified with alcohol and ether gives a blue color upon boiling with strong hydrochloric acid. 6. The lead sulphid reaction. The proteid solution is boiled with a solution of a lead salt made strongly alkaline with soda or potash. A black precipitate or black or brown coloration results according to the amount of proteid. The color is due to the splitting off of sulphur and formation of lead sulphid. It is given, therefore, by the sulphur-containing groups in the proteid molecule. 7. The Molisch reaction. A few drops of an alcoholic solution of a- naphthol are added to the proteid solution and then strong sul- phuric acid. A violet color is obtained. This reaction is given by the carbohydrate grouping in the proteid molecule. The strong acid forms f urfurol from this group, which then reacts with the naph- thol. The reaction is not given by those proteids that do not con- tain a carbohydrate group. Classification of the Proteids. — No classification of the proteids has been proposed which is entirely satisfactory. Eventually a classification will be obtained based upon the chemical structure of the various proteids, but our knowledge at present is much too incomplete for this purpose. We must be content with a less satisfactory system based upon empirical reac- tions which have gradually been recognized in the course of physiological investigations. Albumins. Globulins. Albuminates or derived albumins. I. Simple proteids I Nucleo-albumins (phosphoproteids). \ Proteoses and peptones. Histons. Protamins. Coagulated proteids. 878 APPENDIX. {Chromoproteids (hemoglobin, hemocyanin, etc.). Glucoproteids. Nucleoproteids. r Keratin. III. Albuminoids (pro- i Elastin. teid-like bodies) 1 Collagen (gelatin). v Reticulin. The Albumins. — In addition to the albumins found in the cellular tis- sues, the cell albumins, the conspicuous examples of this group are serum- albumin, milk-albumin (lactalbumin), and egg-albumin (ovalbumin). They are characterized as a class by the fact that they are coagulable by heat iii solutions with a neutral or acid reaction, and are soluble in water free from salts. In accordance with the latter part of this definition they are not precipitated by dialysis. They are precipitated from their solutions with more difficulty by saturation with neutral salts, ammonium sulphate, than the globulins with which they are usually associated. Empirically as re- gards the liquids of the body it is stated that they require more than half saturation with ammonium sulphate for precipitation (see section on Blood). All three albumins referred to here may be obtained in crystallized form. They are not precipitated by saturation with sodium chlorid or magnesium sulphate unless the solution is made acid. They are rich in sulphur, con- taining from 1.6 to 2.2 per cent. The Globulins. — Proteids belonging to this group are found in the cell tissues together with albumins. The forms that have been most studied are serum-globulin (paraglobulin) and fibrinogen (blood, lymph, and transu- data), milk-globulin (lactoglobulin), and egg-globulin. As contrasted with the albumins, they are coagulable by heat, but are not soluble in water free from salts. In consequence of this last property they are precipitated by dialysis. This reaction is not distinctive, however, as the precipitation is not complete. Some of the so-called globulin remains in solution after the salts have been removed as completely as possible by dialysis. They are also precipitated partially from their dilute solutions by the addition of weak acids or by a stream of carbon dioxid Practically they are isolated from accompanying albumins by precipitation with neutral salts. In neu- tral solutions the globulins are completely precipitated by saturation with magnesium sulphate or half saturation with ammonium sulphate. In the blood several different forms of globulin are distinguished by the degree of saturation with ammonium sulphate necessary for their precipitation (see Blood). The separations made by this method are not, however, satisfac- tory. Nor, indeed, is the separation between globulins and albumins alto- gether satisfactory. It would seem that these proteids are so closely related that distinctive reactions are difficult to obtain on account of the existence of forms intermediate between the extremes that are used as types. Albuminates or Derived Albumins. — Since the albumins and globulins occur normally in the tissues and liquids of the body, they are frequently designated as the native proteids. The albuminates, on the contrary, are derived from these native proteids by the action of strong acids or alkalies. We distinguish, therefore, acid and alkali albuminates. They are nearly insoluble in water or in water containing small amounts of neutral salts. They are readily soluble in dilute acid or alkaline solutions; are not pre- cipitated by heat, but are precipitated readily upon neutralization. Nucleo-albumins (Phosphoproteids). — This name may be applied to a group of native proteids which are characterized by containing phos- phorus, such as casein of milk, vitellin from the yolk of the egg, and va- rious similar proteids found in the cytoplasm of the cellular elements. They are often classed with or confounded with the nucleoproteids (or nucleo- albumins) found in the nuclei of the cells. These latter belong to the com- pound proteids and on decomposition yield nucleic acid or its split products, the purin bases, etc., whereas the group now under consideration gives no such result, yielding on decomposition so-called pseudonuclein or paranuclein,. which is not a nuclein at all, but a phosphorus-containing proteid body. PROTEIDS AND THEIR CLASSIFICATION. 879 It is very desirable to select for them another name than nucleo-albumin, since they contain no nuclein. Cohnheim suggests the name of phospho- globulins or phosphoproteids. In addition to phosphorus these proteids. usually contain some iron, but the amount is so small that it can scarcely be present as a constituent part of the molecule. It seems more probable that it is an impurity. Casein contains from 0.8 to 0.9 per cent, of phos- phorus; vitellin, 1.31 per cent, of phosphorus and only 12 per cent, of nitro- gen. These proteids have acid properties. They are not soluble in water, but are very readily soluble in dilute alkalies. Their solutions are not coagu- lated by heat. They show the usual proteid reactions and give the ordi- nary end-products of hydrolytic cleavage upon digestion with proteolytic enzymes or boiling with dilute acid. In the casein it has been shown satisfactorily that no carbohydrate grouping is present in the molecule. Proteoses and Peptones. — These names are given to the intermediate proteids formed in the process of digestion by the proteolytic enzymes. They may also be obtained by the hydrolytic action of acids or alkalies or by the action of putrefactive organisms. If further hydrolyzed they split into some of the amido-acids already referred to as constituting the end-products of proteid hydrolysis. Since they are obtained most readily in peptic and tryptic digestion, they have been supposed to be especially important in the absorption of the proteid foods. They consist of smaller and more sol- uble molecules than those of the proteids from which they are derived. Many different kinds of proteoses have been described, but it is difficult to assign absolutely distinctive characteristics to these compounds, since they form a series of products intermediate between the original proteid and the stage of peptones. The so-called peptones are characterized by their solu- bility and the facts that they are not coagulated by heat and are not pre- cipitated by complete saturation with ammonium sulphate. They give the biuret (red) reaction as well as the xanthoproteic and Millon's reaction, but are not precipitated by the mineral acids, trichloracetic acid, picric acid, acetic acid and potassium ferrocyanid, etc. They are precipitated by phos- photungstic or phosphomolybdic acid. The proteoses in general are dis- tinguished by the following reactions: They are not coagulated by heat; they are precipitated from their solutions by the addition of acetic acid and potassium ferrocyanid ; they are precipitated by nitric acid and this precip- itate dissolves on warming and reappears on cooling; they are precipitated by saturation with ammonium sulphate and by all the so-called alkaloidal reagents, — namely, phosphotungstic acid, trichloracetic acid, picric acid, tannic acid, etc. The proteoses form a group the members of which are variously named. It is customary to speak of the primary proteoses (pro- toproteose and heteroproteose) as distinguished from the secondary pro- teoses (deuteroproteoses A, B, C). The former resemble more the native proteids, while the latter approach the peptones. The distinction between the primary and secondary proteoses is made chiefly upon the ease of pre- cipitation. For example, the primary proteoses are precipitated by a lower degree of saturation with ammonium sulphate and by nitric acid alone, whereas the secondary proteoses require previous saturation with a salt (sodium chlorid) before nitric acid will precipitate them, and they are precip- itated by saturation with ammonium sulphate with much more difficulty, some of them only after making the reaction acid. Protamins and Histons. — The protamins have been obtained (Miescher- Kossel) from the heads of the spermatozoa in fishes, in which they exist in combination with nucleic acid. They differ considerably in the spermatozoa of different animals, and are therefore designated according to the zoological name of the fish from which they arise, as salmin, sturin, clupein, scom- brin, etc. They show a biuret reaction, but in most cases fail to give Millon's reaction. On hydrolysis they give some, but relatively few, of the usual split products of proteids, and these largely the so-called diamido-bodies (arginin, histidin, lysin) rather than the monamido-acids. Some of the latter may occur, however, such as alanin, serin, amidovalerianic or a-pyrrollidin-car- bonic acid. The protamins all give an alkaline reaction, form salts with acids, and are precipitated easily. Their molecular structure is relatively simple. APPENDIX. The molecule contains no sulphur also by its large percentage of nitrogen. Protamin must be regarded as the simplest form of proteid occurring normally in the animal body, a proteid in which many of the groupings, such as cystin, tyro- sin, carbohydrates, found in the usual proteid molecule are entirely lack- ing and in which the basic groupings (arginin) predominate. The histons form a series of compounds intermediate in many ways between the pro- tamins and the usual proteids. The reaction usually considered as char- acteristic of the class is that they are precipitated by ammonia. They are precipitated also by the alkaloidal reagents — e. g., phosphotungstic acid — in neutral solutions. Ordinary proteids give a precipitate with these reagents only in acid solutions, while the protamins give one even in alkaline solutions. Protamins, histons, and the usual proteids form a series, therefore, in which the basic reaction is less and less marked. The best known of the histons is the globin obtained from hemoglobin; another form has been obtained from the nucleohiston in the white corpuscles, from the spermatozoa of mackerel (scombron), codfish (gadushiston), sea-urchin (arbacin), and frog (lotahiston). They do not occur free in the liquids or tissues of the body, but in combination, as in the case of hemoglobin. They give the biuret reaction, a faint Millon reaction, and also respond to the tests for sulphur. The products obtained by their hydrolytic cleavage are much more numer- ous than in the case of the protamins, — a fact which would indicate that their molecular structure is correspondingly more complex. The Compound Proteids. — The chromoproteids may be denned as consisting of a simple proteid in combination with a pigment grouping such as occurs in the case of hemoglobin. A number of such compounds are known, hemoglobin, hemocyanin, hemerythrin, chlorocruorin, — all char- acterized physiologically by the fact that they serve to transport oxygen from the air or water to the tissues. On boiling, heating with alkalies or acids, etc., they readily decompose into their constituent parts (see Blood). Glucoproteids are compounds of a carbohydrate group with a simple proteid. Numerous bodies have been put in this class; some of them contain phos- phorus (phosphoglucoproteids). Those free from phosphorus fall into two divisions: one, the mucins, which on decomposition yield the carbohydrate group in the form of an amido-sugar (glucosamin) , and one, the chondropro- teids, found in the connective tissues and in the pathological substance known as amyloid, which yield their carbohydrate group in the form of chondroitin- sulphuric acid (CjgH^NSOjy) . True mucin is obtained from the secretion of the salivary glands and the mucous glands of the various mucous mem- branes. The nucleoproteids constitute the most interesting of the group of compound proteids. They are recognized as forming an important con- stituent of the cell nuclei. They may be defined as consisting of a compound of simple proteid with a nucleic acid. In the nuclei (head) of spermatozoa the compound, in some cases at least (fishes), contains a nucleic acid and a protamin. In other cases the proteid constituent is more complex. On digestion with pepsin-hydrochloric acid the more complex nucleoproteids split, with the formation, first, of a proteid substance and a simpler nucleo- proteid, richer in phosphorus and designated as a nuclein. On further de- composition this latter yields a nucleic acid. Nucleic acid is therefore the characteristic constituent, and a number of different forms have been des- cribed, all rich in phosphorus, such as thymonucleic acid, salmonnucleic acid, guanylic acid, etc. On hydrolytic decomposition they yield some of the purin bases, — xanthin, guanin, adenin, etc.; some pyrimidin derivatives, — uracil, thymin, cytosin; a carbohydrate group, pentose, levulinic acid; and phosphoric acid. These final decomposition products are characteristic of the true nucleoproteids as distinguished from the phosphorus-containing simple proteids, the nucleo-albumins or phosphoproteids, such as casein. The percentage of phosphorus in the nucleoproteids varies, according to the complexity of the molecule, between 0.5 and 1.6 per cent. The Albuminoids. — This general name is reserved for a group of nitrog- enous bodies found chiefly in the supporting connective tissues of the body, such as the keratin of the epidermis, hairs, etc. ; the elastin of the elastic DIFFUSION AND OSMOSIS. V fa 881 connective tissues; the collagen (ossein) of the white, nbrou^yWf^iyj tn~r\t tissues and bones, from which gelatin is made; and the reticulin of tftosce$ife^*t ular connective tissues of the lymphatic tissues. They resemble the pro- teids closely in general composition, reactions, and end-products of hydro- lytic cleavage, but differ from them evidently in some structural feature, since it has been found by nutritive experiments that they can not be used by the body to replace its proteid tissues (see p. 791). Collagen (gelatin) has the following percentage composition (Chittenden): C, 49.38; H, 6.8; N, 17.97; S, 0.7; O, 25.13. DIFFUSION AND OSMOSIS. In recent years the physical conceptions of the nature of the processes of diffusion and osmosis have changed considerably. As these newer concep- tions have entered largely into current medical literature, it seems advis- able to give a brief description of them for the use of those students of phys- iology who may be unacquainted with the modern nomenclature. The very limited space that can be devoted to the subject forbids anything more than a condensed elementary presentation. For fuller information refer- ence must be made to special treatises.* Diffusion, Dialysis, and Osmosis. — When two gases are brought into contact a homogeneous mixture of the two is soon obtained. This inter- penetration of the gases is spoken of as diffusion, and it is due to the con- tinual movements of the gaseous molecules to and fro within the limits of the confining space. So also when two miscible liquids or solutions are brought into contact a diffusion occurs for the same reason, the movements of the molecules finally effecting a homogeneous mixture. If the two liquids happen to be separated by a membrane diffusion will still occur, provided the membrane is permeable to the liquid molecules, and in time the liquids on the two sides will be mixtures having a uniform composition. Not only water molecules, but the molecules of many substances in solution, such as sugar, may pass to and fro through membranes, so that two liquids sepa- rated from each other by an intervening membrane and originally unlike in composition may finally, by the act of diffusion, come to have the same composition. Diffusion of this kind through a membrane is frequently spoken of as dialysis or osmosis. In the body we deal with aqueous solu- tions of various substances that are separated from each other by living membranes, such as the walls of the blood capillaries or of the alimentary canal, and the laws of diffusion through membranes are of immediate im- portance in explaining the passage of water and dissolved substances through these living septa. In aqueous solutions such as we have in the body we must take into account the movements of the molecules of the solvent, water, as well as of the substances dissolved. These latter may have different de- grees of diffusibility as compared with one another or with the water mole- cules, and it frequently happens that a membrane that is permeable to water molecules is less permeable or even impermeable to the molecules of the substances in solution. For this reason the diffusion stream of water and of the dissolved substances may be differentiated, as it were, to a greater or less extent. In recent years it has become customary to limit the term osmosis to the stream of water molecules passing through a membrane, while the term dialysis, or diffusion, is applied to the passage of the mole- cules of the substances in solution. The osmotic stream of water under vary- ing conditions is especially important, and in connection with this process it is necessary to define the term osmotic pressure as applied to solutions. Osmotic Pressure. — If we imagine two masses of water separated by a permeable membrane, we can readily understand that as many water mole- * Consult Cohen, " Phvsical Chemistry for Physicians and Biologists, " translated by Fischer, 1903. H. C. Jones, " The Theory of Electrolytic Dis- sociation," 1900; "Diffusion, Osmosis, and Filtration," by E. W. Reid, in Schafer's "Text-book of Physiology," 1898. 56 882 APPENDIX cules will pass through from one side as from the other; the two streams, in fact, will neutralize each other, and the volumes of the two masses of water will remain unchanged. The movement of the water molecules in this case is not actually observed, but it is assumed to take place on the theory that the liquid molecules are continually in motion and that the membrane, being permeable, offers no obstacle to their movements. If. now, on one side of the membrane we place a solution of some crystalloid substance, such as common salt, and on the other side pure water, then it will be found that an excess of water will pass from the water side to the side containing the solution. In the older terminology it was said that the salt attracted this water, but in the newer theories the same fact is expressed by saying that the salt in solution exerts a certain osmotic pressure, in conse- quence of which more water flows from the water side to the side of the solution than in the reverse direction. As a matter of experiment it is found that the osmotic pressure varies with the amount of the substance in solu- tion. If in experiments of this kind a semipermeable membrane is chosen — that is, a membrane that is permeable to the water molecules, but not to the molecules of the substance in solution — the stream of water to the side of the crystalloid will continue until the hydrostatic pressure on this side reaches a certain point, and the hydrostatic pressure thus caused may be taken as a measure of the osmotic pressure exerted by the substance in solu- tion. Under these conditions it can be shown that the osmotic pressure is proportional to the concentration of the solution, or, in other words, to the number of molecules (and ions) of the crystalloid in solution. As a matter of fact, most of the membranes that we have to deal with in the body are only approximately semipermeable — that is, while they are readily permeable to water molecules, they are also permeable, although with more or less difficulty, to the substances in solution. In such cases we get an osmotic stream of water to the side of the dissolved crystalloid, but at the same time the molecules of the latter pass to some extent through the mem- brane, by diffusion, to the other side. In course of time, therefore, the dissolved crystalloid will be equally distributed on the two sides of the mem- brane, the osmotic pressure on both sides will become equal, and osmosis of the water will cease to be apparent, since it is equal in the two directions. All substances in true solution are capable of exerting osmotic pressure, and the important discovery has been made that the osmotic pressure, meas- ured in terms of atmospheres or the pressure of a column of water or mer- cury, is equal to the gas pressure that would be exerted by a number of molecules of gas equal to that of the crystalloid in solution, if confined within the same space and kept at the same temperature.* A perfectly satisfactory explanation of the nature of osmotic pressure has not been furnished. We must be content to use the term to express the fact described. It is a matter of great importance to measure the osmotic pressures of various solutions. As was stated above, this measurement can be made for any solution pro- vided a really semipermeable membrane is constructed. As a matter of fact, however, the use of such membranes has not been general. In actual experiments other methods have been employed, and a brief statement of a theoretical and a practical method of arriving at the value of osmotic pressures may be of service in further illustrating the meaning of the term. Before stating these methods it becomes necessary to define two terms — namely, electrolytes and gram-molecular solutions — that are much used in this connection. Electrolytes. — The molecules of many substances when brought into * The interesting researches of Morse and Frazer (" The American Chemi- cal Journal," 34, 1, 1905), who have succeeded in making semipermeable membranes in such a form as may be used for determining directly the os- motic pressures of concentrated (normal) solutions, have shown that this law is not accurately stated. The actual pressure is that which would be exerted if the particles in solution were gasified at the same temperature and kept to the volume of the pure solvent used (water), instead of the volume of the entire solution. DIFFUSION AND OSMOSIS. 883 a state of solution are believed to be dissociated into two or more parts, known as ions. The completeness of the dissociation varies with the sub- stance used, and for any one substance with the degree of dilution. Roughly speaking, the greater the dilution, the more nearly complete is the dissocia- tion. The ions liberated by this act of dissociation are charged with elec- tricity, and when an electrical current is led into the solution it is conducted by the movements of the ions. The molecules of perfectly pure water undergo almost no dissociation, and water therefore does not appreciably conduct the electrical current. If some NaCl is dissolved in water, a certain num- ber of its molecules become dissociated into a Na ion charged positively with electricity and a CI ion charged negatively, and the solution becomes a conductor of the electrical current. Substances that exhibit this property of dissociation are known as electrolytes, to distinguish them from other soluble substances, such as sugar, that do not dissociate in solution and therefore do not conduct the electrical current. Speaking generally, it may be said that all salts, bases, and acids belong to the group of electrolytes. The conception of electrolytes is very important for the reason that the act of dissociation obviously increases the number of particles moving in the solution and thereby increases the osmotic pressure, since it has been found experimentally that, so far as osmotic pressures are concerned, an ion plays the same part as a molecule. It follows, therefore, that the osmotic pressure of any given electrolyte in solution is increased in proportion to the degree to which it is dissociated. As the liquids of the body contain electrolytes in solution it becomes necessary, in estimating their osmotic pressure, to take this fact into consideration. Gram-molecular Solutions. — The concentration of a given substance in solution may be stated by the usual method of percentages, but from the standpoint of osmotic pressure a more convenient method is the use of the unit known as a gram-molecular solution. A gram-molecule of any sub- stance is a quantity in grams of the substance equal to its molecular weight, while a gram-molecular solution is one containing a gram-molecule of the substance to a liter of the solution. Thus, a gram-molecular solution of sodium chlorid is one containing 58.5 gms. (Na, 23; CI, 35.5) of the salt to a liter, while a gram-molecular solution of cane-sugar contains 342 gms. (Cj-jH^Oh) to a liter. Similarly a gram-molecule of H is 2 gms. by weight of this gas, and if this weight of H were compressed to the volume of a liter it would be comparable to a gram-molecular solution. Since the weight of a molecule of H is to the weight of a molecule of cane-sugar as 2 is to 342, it follows that a liter containing 2 gms. of H contains the same number of molecules of H in it as a liter of solution containing 342 gms. of sugar has of sugar molecules. On the assumption that a molecule in solution exerts an osmotic pressure that is exactly equal to the gas-pressure exerted by a gas molecule moving in the same space and at the same temperature, we are justified in saying that the osmotic pressure of a gram-molecular solu- tion of cane-sugar, or of any other substance that is not an electrolyte, is equal to the gas-pressure of 2 gms. of H when compressed to the volume of 1 liter. This fact gives a means of calculating the osmotic pressure of solutions in certain cases according to the following method: Calculation of the Osmotic Pressure of Solutions. — To illustrate this method we may take a simple problem such as the determination of the osmotic pressure of a 1 per cent, solution of cane-sugar. One gm. of H at atmospheric pressure occupies a volume of 11.16 liters; 2 gms. of H, there- fore, under the same conditions will occupy a volume of 22.32 liters. A gram-molecule of H — that is, 2 gms. of H — when brought to the volume of 1 liter will exert a gas-pressure equal to that of 22.32 liters compressed to 1 liter — that is, a pressure of 22.32 atmospheres. A gram-molecular solu- tion of cane-sugar, since it contains the same number of molecules in a liter, must therefore exert an osmotic pressure equal to 22.32 atmospheres. A 1 per cent, solution of cane-sugar contains, however, only 10 gms. of sugar to a liter; hence the osmotic pressure of the sugar in such a solution will be 34^ of 22.32 atmospheres, or 0.65 of an atmosphere, which in terms of 884 APPENDIX. a column of mercury gives 760 X 0.65 = 494 mms. This figure expresses the osmotic pressure of a 1 per cent, solution of cane-sugar when dialyzed against pure water through a membrane impermeable to the sugar molecules. In such an experiment water would pass to the sugar side until the hydro- static pressure on this side was increased by an amount equal to the pres- sure of a column of mercury 494 mms. high. Certain additional calculations that it is necessary to make for the temperature of the solution need not be specified in this connection. If, however, we wish to apply this method to the calculation of the osmotic pressure of a given solution of an electro- lyte, it is necessary first to ascertain the degree of dissociation of the electro- lyte into its ions, since, as was said above, dissociation increases the num- ber of parts in solution and to the same extent increases osmotic pressure. In the body the liquids that concern us contain a variety of substances in solution, electrolytes as well as non-electrolytes. In order, therefore, to calculate the osmotic pressure of such complex solutions it is necessary to ascertain the amount of each substance present, and, in the case of electro- lytes, the degree of dissociation. Under experimental conditions such a calculation is practically impossible, and recourse must be had to other methods. One of the simplest and most easily applied of these methods is the determination of the freezing point of the solution. Determination of Osmotic Pressure by Means of the Freezing Point. — This method depends upon the fact that the freezing point of water is low- . ered by substances in solution, and it has been discovered that the amount of lowering is proportional to the number of parts (molecules and ions) present in the solution. Since the osmotic pressure is also proportional to the number of parts in solution, it is convenient to take the lowering of the freezing point of a solution as an index or measure of its osmotic pressure. In practice a simple apparatus (Beckmann's apparatus) is used, consisting essentially of a very delicate and adjustable differential thermometer. By means of this instrument the freezing point of pure water is first ascertained upon the empirical scale of the thermometer. The freezing point of the solution under examination is then determined, and the number of degrees or fractions of a degree by which its freezing point is lower than that of pure water is noted. The lowering of the freezing point in degrees centigrade is expressed usually by the symbol A- For example, mammalian blood- serum gives A = 0.56° C. A 6.95 per cent, solution of NaCl gives the same A ; hence the two solutions exert the same osmotic pressure, or, to put it in another way, a 0.95 per cent, solution of NaCl is isotonic or isosmotic with mammalian serum. The A of any given solution may be expressed in terms of a gram-molecular solution by dividing it by the constant 1.87, since a gram-molecular solution of a non-electrolyte is known to lower the freezing point 1.87° C. Thus, if blood-serum gives A = 0.56° C.,its concentration in terms of a gram-molecular solution will be —. or o.3. In other words, blood-serum has 0.3 of the osmotic pressure exerted by a gram-molecular solution of a non-electrolyte, — that is, 22.32 X 0.3, or 6.696 atmospheres. Remarks upon the Application of the Foregoing Facts in Physiol- ogy.— In the body water and substances in solution are continually pass- ing through membranes, — for example, in the production of lymph, in the absorption of water and digested foodstuffs from the alimentary canal, in the nutritive exchanges between the tissue elements and the blood or lymph, in the production of the various secretions, and so on. In these cases it is a matter of the greatest difficulty to give a satisfactory explanation of the forces controlling the flow to and fro of the water and dissolved substances, but there can be little doubt that in all of them the physical forces of fil- tration, diffusion, and osmosis take an important part. Whatever can be learned, therefore, concerning these processes must in the end have an im- portant bearing upon the explanation of the nutritive exchanges between the blood and tissues. Some additional facts may be mentioned to indicate the applications that are made of these processes in explaining physiological phenomena. Osmotic Pressure of Proteids. — The osmotic pressure exerted by crys- DIFFUSION AND OSMOSIS. 885 talloids, such as the ordinary soluble salts, is, as we have seen, very con- siderable, but the ready diffusibility of most of these salts through animal membranes limits very materially their influence upon the flow of water in the body. Thus, if we should inject a strong solution of common salt directly into the blood-vessels, the first effect would be the setting up of an osmotic stream from the tissues to the blood and the production of a condition of hydremic plethora within the blood-vessels. The salt, however, would soon diffuse out into the tissues, and to the degree that this occurred its effect in diluting the blood would tend to diminish because the part of the salt that got into the extravascular lymph spaces would now exert an osmotic press- ure in the opposite direction, drawing water from the blood. This fact, together with the further fact that an excess of salts in the body is soon re- moved by the excretory organs, gives to such substances a smaller influence in directing the water stream than would at first be supposed when the inten- sity of their osmotic action is considered. In addition to the crystalloids the liquids of our bodies contain also a certain amount of proteid, the blood, especially, containing over 6 per cent, of this substance. It has been gen- erally assumed that proteids in solution exert little or no osmotic pressure, but Starling* and others have claimed, on the contrary, that they exert a distinct, although small, osmotic pressure, and it is possible that this fact is of special importance in absorption, because the proteids do not diffuse or diffuse with great difficulty, and their effect remains, therefore, so to speak, as a permanent factor. According to Starling, the osmotic pressure exerted by the proteids of serum is equal to about 30 mms. of mercury. That the osmotic pressure of the serum proteids is so small is not sur- prising if we remember the very high molecular weight of this substance. In serum the proteids are present in a concentration of about 7 per cent., but owing to their large molecular weight comparatively few proteid mole- cules are present in a solution of this concentration; and, assuming that the dissolved proteid follows the laws discovered for crystalloids, its osmotic pressure would depend upon the number of molecules in solution. By means of this weak, but constant, osmotic pressure of the indiffusible pro- teid it is possible to explain theoretically the fact that an isotonic or even a hypertonic solution of diffusible crystalloid may be completely absorbed by the blood from the peritoneal cavity. Reidt has given evidence which indicates that pure proteids exert no osmotic pressure; that as they occur in the body liquids they are combined or mixed with certain substances to which the feeble osmotic pressure formerly attributed to the proteids really belongs. Since these unknown substances are themselves indiffusible, the arguments just used still hold for the conditions in the body. Isotonic, Hypertonic, and Hypotonic Solutions. — In physiology the osmotic pressures exerted by various solutions are compared usually with that of the blood-serum. In this sense an isotonic or isosmotic solution is one having an osmotic pressure equal to that of serum, a hypertonic or hy- perosmotic solution is one whose osmotic pressure exceeds that of serum, and a hypotonic or hyposmotic solution is one whose osmotic pressure is less than that of serum. Diffusion, or Dialysis, of Soluble Constituents. — If two liquids of unequal concentration in a given constituent are separated by a membrane entirely permeable to the dissolved molecules of the substance, a greater number of these molecules will pass over from the more concentrated to the less concentrated side, and in time the composition will be the same on the two sides of the membrane. Diffusion of soluble constituents continually takes place, therefore, from the points of greater concentration to those of less, and this may happen quite independently of the direction of the osmotic stream of water. If, for instance, a 0.9 per cent, solution of sodium chlorid is injected into the peritoneal cavity, it will enter into diffusion relations with the blood in the blood-vessels; its concentration in sodium chlorid being greater than that of the blood, the excess will tend to pass into the * "Journal of Physiology," 24, 317, 1899. t Reid, "Journal of Physiology," 1905. 886 APPENDIX. blood, while sodium carbonate, urea, sugar, and other soluble crystalloidal substances will pass from the blood into the salt solution in the peritoneal cavity. Through the action of this process of diffusion we can understand how certain constituents of the blood may pass to the tissues of various glands in amounts greater than can be explained if we supposed that the lymph of these tissues is derived solely by filtration from the blood-plasma. An- other important conception in this connection is the possibility that the capillary walls may be permeable in different degrees to the various soluble constituents of the blood, and furthermore the possibility that the permea- bility of the capillary walls may vary in different organs. With regard to the first possibility it has been shown that the blood capillaries are more permeable to the urea molecules than to sugar or NaCl. With the aid of these facts it is possible to explain in large measure the transportation of material from the blood to the tissues, and vice versa. For example, to follow a line of reasoning used by Roth, we may suppose that the functional activity of the tissue elements is attended by a consumption of material which in turn is made good by the dissolved molecules in the tissue lymph. The concentration of the latter is thereby lowered, and in consequence a diffu- sion stream of these substances is set up with the more concentrated blood. In this way, by diffusion, a constant supply of dissolved material is kept in motion from the blood to the tissue elements. On the other hand, the functional activity of the tissue elements is accompanied by a breaking down of the complex proteid molecule, with the formation of simpler, more stable molecules of crystalloid character, such as the sulphates, phosphates, and urea or some precursor of urea. As these bodies pass into the tissue lymph they tend to increase its concentration, and thus by the greater osmotic pressure which they exert serve to attract water from the blood to the lymph, forming one efficient factor in the production of lymph. On the other hand, as these substances accumulate in the lymph to a concentration greater than that possessed by the same substances in the blood, they will diffuse toward the blood. By this means the waste products of activity are drawn off to the blood, from which, in turn, they are removed by the action of the excretory organs. Diffusion of Proteids. — This simple explanation on purely physical grounds of the flow of material between the blood and the tissues can only be applied, however, at present to the diffusible crystalloids, such as the salts, urea, and sugar. The proteids of the blood, which are supposed to be so important for the nutrition of the tissues, are practically indiffusible, so far as we know. It is difficult to explain their passage from the blood through the capillary walls into the lymph. Provisionally it may be assumed that this passage is due to filtration. The blood-plasma in the capillaries is under a slightly higher pressure than the lymph of the tissues, and this higher pressure tends to squeeze the blood constituents, including the proteid, through the capillary walls. This explanation, however, can not be said to be satisfactory; and in this respect the purely physical theory of lymph formation waits upon a clearer knowledge of the nature of the nutri- tive proteids and their relations to the capillary wall (see Lymph, p. 427). INDEX. Abdominal respiration, 572 type of respiration, 576 Absolute power of muscle, 36 Absorption coefficient, 600 in large intestine, 717 in small intestine, 712 in stomach, 698 of carbohydrates, 713 of fats, 714 of proteids, 716 spectra of hemoglobin, 394 Acapnia, 625, 630 Accelerator center for heart, 525 nerves, effect of , on heart rate, 527 of heart, 522 reflex stimulation of, 524 Accessory thyroids, 772 Accommodation in eye, limits of, 292 mechanism of, 289 reflex, 301 Achromatic series of visual sensa- tions, 320 Achroodextrin, 680 Acid albumin in gastric digestion, 693 ami do-acetic, 706, 874 amidocaproic, 706, 874 amidoglutaric, 874 ami do-oxy propionic, 874 amidopropionic, 874 amidosuccinic, 706, 874, amidothiolactic, 874 amidovalerianic, 706, 874 cholic, 725 conjugated sulphuric, 760 diamidocaproic, 874 glutaminic, 706, 874 glycocholic, 725 guanidinamidovalerianic, 874 hippuric, 759 lactic, 60 oxypnenylamidopropionic, 874 phenylamidopropionic, 874 phosphocarnic, 60 pyrrolidin-carbonic, 874 taurocholic, 725 Acromegaly, 778 Action current, diphasic, 98 in heart, 480 in muscle, 96 in nerve, 96 Action current in retina, 309 monophasic, 98 relation of, to contraction wave, 100 to nerve impulse, 100 Adamkiewicz's reaction for proteids, 877 Addison's disease, 775 Adenase, 664 Adenin, 61, 757 Adrenal bodies, functions of, 775 Adrenalin, 776 Aerial perspective, 345 Aerotonometer, 601 Afferent fibers in cranial nerves, 79 position of, in posterior roots, 77 nerve fibers, 75 After-images in eye, 323 Air, effect of variations in, on res- pirations, 627 Alanin, 706, 874 Albumin, properties of, 878 (see also Proteid) Albuminates, properties of, 878 Albuminoids, nutritive value of, 791 properties of, 880 Albumon, 414 Alcohol as food, 808 effect of, on gastric absorption, 699 physiological action of, 805 Alimentary canal, movements of, 634 glycosuria, 714 principles of food, 654 Altitude, effect of, on red corpus- cles, 403 Amboceptors, 388 Ametropia, 295 Amido-acetic acid, 706, 874 Amido-acids as part of proteid mole- cule, 706 Amidocaproic acid, 706, 874 Amidoglutaric acid, 874 Amido-oxypropionic acid, 874 Amidopropionic acid, 706, 874 Amidosuccinic acid, 706, 874 Amidothiolactic acid, 874 Amidovalerianic acid, 706, 874 Ammonia compounds in urine, 752 salts, relation of, to urea, 755 887 888 INDEX. Ammonium carbamate as precursor of urea, 754 carbonate as precursor of urea, 737 Amylolytic enzyme, 662 Amylopsin, 708 Anacrotic pulse, 474, 475 Anaglyph, 347 Anelectrotonic currents, 101 Anelectrotonus, 83 Anisotropous substance in muscle, 19 Anode as stimulus to muscle, 82 to nerve, 82 physical, 89 physiological, 89 Anoxemia, 628 Anterior commissure, 213 marginal bundle, spinal cord, 170 roots of spinal nerves, 77 Antidromic impulses in vasodilator nerve fibers, 78, 548 Antilytic secretion, 677 Antiparalytic secretion, 677 Antipeptone, 705 Antithrombin, 419 Antrum pylori of stomach, 639 Anus praeternaturalis, 717 Apex beat of heart, 482 Aphasia, motor, 204 sensory, 206 Apnea, 623 Arginase, 756 Arginin, 707, 863, 874 Argyll Robertson pupil, 302 Arterial pressure (see Circulation) Artificial muscle of Engelmann, 69 respiration, 581 stimuli of nerve fibers, 80 Ascending paths in spinal cord, 161 Aspartic acid, 706, 874 Asphyxia, effect of, on heart-rate, 529 Aspiratory action of thorax, 587 Association areas in brain, 207, 209 system of fibers in cerebrum, 175 Astereognosis, 189 Astigmatism, 297 Atropin, action of, on heart, 520 on iris, 303 on salivary secretion, 676 on sweat secretion, 768 Auditory center in cortex, 198 nerve, course of, in brain, 199 effect of section of, 375 sensations (see Ear) stria-, 201 Auricle (see Heart) Auriculoventricular muscle bundle, 479 effect of compressing, 508 Auscultation, 486 Autolysis, 835 Autolytic enzymes, 664 Automaticity of heart beat, 500 Autonomic fibers from brain, 234 from sacral cord, 236 from spinal cord, 233 nerve fibers, normal stimulation of, 236 nervous system, general relations of, 232 Axial stream in arteries, 435 in veins, 435 Axon reflexes, 142 Bacterial action in intestines, 718 Balance experiments in nutrition, 785 Balloon ascensions, effect of, 630 Banting diet, 800 Barometric pressures, effect of, 629 Bartholin, duct of, 666 Basilar membrane, function of, 367 Bathmotropic nerve fibers to heart, 516 Bell-Magendie law, 77 Bilateral representation in motor areas, 186 Bile, composition of, 722 curve of secretion of, 729 effect of occluding bile ducts, 730 ejection of, into duodenum, 728 functions of, summary, 730 quantity of, 722 secretion of, 727 Bile-acids, composition of, 725 Bile-pigments, 400, 724 Bile-salts, importance of, in fat digestion, 715 Bilirubin, 724 Biliverdin, 724 Binocular perspective, 346, 349 vision, 338 horopter in, 343 struggle of visual fields in, 344 value of, in judgments of dis- tance, 344 in judgments of size, 344 Births, ratio of male and female, 869 Biuret reaction for proteids, 877 Bladder, urinary, movements of, 762 Blind spot in eye, 308 Blood, blood-plates of, physiology of, 407 circulation of (see Circulation) coagulation of, 414 condition of carbon dioxid in, 604 of nitrogen in, 602 curve of dissociation of oxyhemo- globin, 603 defibrinated, 382 gases of, 596 general properties of, 381 INDEX. 889 Blood, hemoglobin in, 385 amount, 389 nature, 389 hemolysis of red corpuscles, 385 histological structure of, 381 leucocytes of, 404 nucleoproteid in. 414 osmotic pressure of, 386 proteids of, 410, 413 reaction of, 382 reasons for not clotting in vessels, 419 red corpuscles of, 384 fate of, 401 origin of, 401 regeneration of, after hemorrhage, 424 Blood-plasma, composition of, 408 Blood-plates, effect of hemorrhage upon, 425 part taken by, in coagulation, 417 properties of, 406 Blood-pressure, effect of menstrua- tion upon, 846 in brain, 556 relation of, to heart rate, 527 respiratory waves of, 588 Traube-Hering waves of, 544 Blood-serum, 382, 409 specific gravity of, 383 total quantity of, 423 transfusion of, 426 Body equilibrium in nutrition, 785 sense area, 188 temperature in man, 821 Bowman's theory of urinary secre- tion, 743 Brain, association areas of, 207 auditory center in, 198 center for taste, 204 for olfaction, 202 centers affected in aphasia, 204 development of cortical areas, 210 effect of variations in arterial pressure upon, 557 intracranial pressure in, 556 localization of function in, 181 motor areas of, 183 regulation of circulation in, 559 sensory areas of, 188 vascular supply of, 552 Brightness in visual sensations, 319 Bronchi, capacity of, 581 constrictor and dilator fibers of, 625 Buffy coat of blood, 415 Burdach, column of, 162 Caffein, 757, 805 Caisson disease, 629 Calcarine fissure, relation of, to vision, 195 Calcium salts, effect of, on heart, 501 importance of, in clotting of blood, 416 in curdling of milk, 696 nutritive value of, 803 of milk, 429 Calorie, definition of, 35, 823 Calorimeters, varieties of, 824 Calorimetric equivalent, 825 measurements, 828 Calorimetry, indirect, 827 principles of, 823 Cannula, washout, 443 Capillary blood-pressure, 452 electrometer, construction of, 94 Carbohydrates, absorption of, 713 as glycogen formers, 732 as source of body fat, 798 fate of, in body, 793 fermentation of, in intestine, 714 general functions of, 794 of muscle, 59 supply of, in body, 793 Carbon dioxid, action on respira- tory center, 621 condition of, in blood, 604 effect of, in inspired air, 594, 629 excretion of, through skin, 770 in balance experiments, 785 in saliva, 669 production of, in muscle, 62 tension of, in alveolar air, 607 in arterial blood, 608 in tissues, 608 in venous blood, 607 equilibrium, 785 monoxid hemoglobin, 391 Cardiac cycle, 488 glands of stomach, 683 muscle, properties of, 54, 504 nerves (see Heart) Cardiogram, 482 Cardio-inhibitory center, 517, 518 Carnin, 61 Casein, 695, 879 Catalase, 664 Catalysis, 658 Catelectrotonic currents, 101 Catelectrotonus, 83 Cathode as stimulus, 82 physical and physiological, 89 Centers in cerebrum (see Cerebrum) Central field of vision, 313 Centrosome, 848 Cerebellum, ending of spinal tracts in, 164 experimental work upon, 220 general functions of, 219, 223 890 INDEX. Cerebellum, localization of function in, 224 paths connecting with other parts of brain, 218 psychical functions of, 224 structure of, 216 Cerebrospinal liquid, 555 Cerebrum, association areas of, 207 system of fibers in, 175 auditory center in, 198 center for hearing, 198 for olfaction, 202 for vision, 192 centers affected in aphasia, 206 commissural system of fibers in, 175 development of cortical areas in, 210 general physiology of, 171 histology of cortex, 172 localization of function in, 179 motor areas of, 183 projection system of fibers in, 173 results of ablation of, 178 sensory areas of, 188 vasomotor supply of, 552 Cerumen, 770 Chemotaxis, 119 Cheyne-Stokes respiration, 632 Cholesterin, 721, 726, 770 Cholic acid, 725 Cholin, 727 Chorda tympani nerve, 270, 667, 670 Chromatic aberration in eye, 293 visual sensations, 321 Chromatolysis, 121 Chromophile substance in nerve cells, 127 Chromoproteids, 880 Chromosomes, 848, 867 Chronotropic nerve fibers to heart, 516 Chyle fat, 715 Chymosin, 695 Ciliary muscle, action of, in accom- modation, 291 nerves to, 299 Ciliated epithelium, 54 Circulating proteids, 787 Circulation, accessory factors in, 466 as seen under microscope, 434 curve showing pressures in, 452 data concerning pressures in (man), 458 • mean pressures in, 450 diastole and systole of heart, 477 effect of adrenal extracts upon, 776 of heart beat and size of arteries upon, 440 explanation of side-pressure in, 460 of velocity changes in, 439 Circulation, explanation of velocity- pressure in, 460 factors of normal pressure and velocity in, 463 form of pulse wave, 472 general curve of velocities in, 439 general statement of pressure rela- tions, 442 hydrostatic effects upon, 465 importance of elasticity of arteries, 463 in brain, 552, 559 in kidneys, 749 means of determining blood-pres- sure in, 442, 453 velocities in, 435 of lymph, 564 pressures in coronary system, 491 in pulmonary system, 467, 468 pulse pressures in, 446 respiratory waves of pressure in, 588 systole and diastole of heart, 477 systolic, diastolic, and mean pres- sures in, 446 and mean velocities in, 438 time required for complete, 441 Traube-Hering waves of pressure in, 544 velocity in capillaries, 438 in coronary system, 491 in man, 438 in pulmonary system, 467, 468 of pulse wave, 470 Clarke's column, 156 Clotting (see Coagulation) Coagulation, blood, 414 intravascular, 421 means of hastening and retarding, 421 theories of, 415 Cocain, action of, on iris, 303 Cochlea, functions of, 366 sensory epithelium of, 360 Cold spots on skin, 257 Color blindness, 325, 327 contrasts, 324 fusion, 321 saturation, 321 sense of retina, 328 vision, theories of, 331 Combustion equivalents of foods, 815 Comma tract of Schultze, 169 Commissural system of fibers (cere- brum), 175 Common sensations, 256 Compensatory pause in heart-beat, 506 Complemental air, 580 Complementary colors, 322 Compound muscular contractions, 39 Condiments, 656, 804 INDEX. 891 Conduction as physiological property, 72 direction of, in nerve fibers, 107 Conjugate foci, 283 Conjugated sulphates, 759 Contraction, 17, 32, 33 (see also Muscle) Contralateral conduction in cord, 166 Convoluted tubules of kidney, 746 Core-model of nerve, 102 Coronary arteries, effect of occlusion of, 494 vasomotor supply of, 491 Corpora quadrigemina, relation of, to visual apparatus, 193, 197 striata, functions of, 214 Corpus callosum, structure and func- tion, 213 luteum, 840 trapezoideum, 200 Corresponding points of retina, 340 Cortex of cerebrum, general physi- ology of, 175 histology of, 172 Corti, rods of, 367 Costal respiration, 573, 576 Cowper's glands, 861 Cranial nerves, afferent fibers in, 79 efferent fibers of, 79 nuclei of origin of, 226 Cranioscopy, 180 Creatin, 61, 66, 758 Creatinin, 61, 758 Cresol-sulphuric acid, 719, 760 Cretinism, 773 Curve, contraction, of artificial muscle, 70 of plain muscle, 53 of skeletal muscle, 25 dissociation of oxyhemoglobin, 603 extensibility and elasticity of muscle, 19 gastric secretion, 690 intensity of sleep, 240 pancreatic secretion, 702 pressures in vascular system, 452 relations of heart sounds, 487, 488 respiratory movements, 578 secretion of bile, 729 systolic, diastolic, and mean blood- pressures, 447 velocity of blood-flow, 439 visual acuity of retina, 316 .work of muscle, 36 Cycloplegia, 303 Cystin, 726, 874 Cystinuria, 726 Death rigor, chemical changes dur- ing, 66 in muscle, 49 theories of, 872 Defecation, 650 Degeneration in nerve fibers, 118, 120 Deglutition, 634 nervous control of, 638 Deiters's cells in cochlea, 368 Demarcation current, muscle and nerve, 91 Demilunes of salivary glands, 668 Depressor nerve fibers, 540 of heart, 543 Derived albumins, properties of, 878 Descending paths, spinal cord, 167, 169 Deutero-albumoses, 693 Diabetes, 733 mellitus, 780 pancreatic, 780 phloridzin, 795 Dialysis, definition of, 881 Diaphragm, action of, 572 Diaphragmatic respiration, 572 Diastase, 662 discovery of, 657 Diastole, duration of, in heart, 480 Diastolic arterial pressure, 446 method of determining in animals, 447 method of determining, in man, 454 Dicrotic pulse wave, 474 Diet, accessory articles of, 656, 804 average daily, 818 Banting's, 800 effect of reduction of salts in, 802 Dietetics, general principles of, 817 Diffusion circles on retina, 289 definition of, 881 Digestion in large intestine, 717 in small intestine, 700 in stomach, 696 Diopter, definition of, 293 Dioptrics of eye, 282 Diplopia, 342 Direct field of vision, eye, 313 Discord, physiological explanation of, 369 Dissociation of oxyhemoglobin, 603 Diuretics, action of, 748 Dromograph, 437 Dromotropic nerve fibers to heart, 516 Duct of Bartholin, 666 of Rivinus, 666 of Stenson, 666 of Wharton, 666 of Wirsung, 700 Dyspnea, 575, 622 Ear effect of section of auditory nerves, 375 892 INDEX. Ear, effect of stimulation of semi- circular canals, 374 Eustachian tube of, 359 Flourens's experiments on semi- circular canals, 372 functions in analyzing sound waves, 365 of cochlea, 366 of ear-bones, 356 of sacculus, 379 of utriculus, 379 intrinsic muscles of, 358 limits of hearing, 370 position of bones in, 355 projection of auditory sensations, 360 semicircular canals of, 372 sensations of discord, 369 of harmony, 369 sensory epithelium in cochlea. 360 structure of, 353 bones of, 355 theories of functions of semicircu- lar canals of, 375 tympanic membrane of, 354 Eck fistula, 754 Efferent nerve fibers, 75 occurrence in anterior roots, 77 in cranial nerves, 79 Ejaculation of spermatic liquid, 864 Elasticity and extensibility of muscle, 19 Electrical currents during heart beat, 480 Electrodes, non-polarizable, 95 stimulating, 81 Electrolytes, effect of, on osmotic pressure, 882 Electrotonic currents, 101 Electrotonus, 83 Eleventh cranial nerve, nucleus of, 230 Embryo, nutrition of, 852 Endogenous fibers of spinal cord, 162 Energy of muscular contraction, 34 Enterokinase, 704, 711 Entoptic phenomena, 336 Enzymes, adenase, 664 amylopsin, 708 arginase, 756 chemistry of, 664 classification of, 661 definition of, 661 enterokinase, 704, 711 erepsin, 711 general properties of, 663 glycolytic, 782 guanase, 664 historical account of, 657 intracellular, 835 inverting, 711 Enzymes, lipase, 709 of muscle, 61 oxidases, 836 pepsin, 691 peroxidases, 836 ptyalin, 679 rennin, 695 reversible reactions of, 659 specificity of, 661 thrombin, 416 trypsin, 705 Epigenesis, 866 Epinephrin, 776 Erection, physiology of, 863 Erepsin, 664, 711 Ergograph, 45 Erythroblasts, 402 Erythrocytes (see Red corpuscles) Erythrodextrin, 680 Eserin, action of, on iris, 303 Esophoria, 340 Ethereal sulphates, 759 Eupnea, 575 Eustachian tube, 359 Evolution, hypothesis of, in heredity, 866 Excretion, 721 Exogenous fibers in spinal cord, 162 Exophoria, 340 Expiration (see also Respiration) definition of, 571 expiratory center, 619 muscles of, 574 Expired air, composition of, 592 injurious effects of, 593 Extensibility of muscle, 19 External geniculates, 193, 197 Eye, abnormalities in refraction of, 295 accommodation in, 301 mechanism of, 289 action of drugs upon, 303 acuity of vision in, 315 after-images in, 323 as optical instrument, 282 binocular field of vision, 340 perspective, 346 chromatic aberration in, 293 color blindness of, 325 contrasts in, 324 vision in, 321 complementary colors, 322 corresponding points in, 340 dark adapted, 310, 318 diffusion circles in, 289 diplopia in, 342 direct field of vision^ in, 313 entoptic phenomena in, 336 far point of distinct vision, 292 function of cones, 330 of rods, 330 fundamental color sensations, 322 INDEX. 893 Eye, horopter, 343 indirect field of vision in, 313 inversion of image in, 287 light adapted, 310, 318 reflex in, 301 movements of, 338 muscular insufficiency, 339 nature of visual stimuli, 308 near point of distinct vision, 292 nodal point of, 286 ophthalmoscopic examination of, 305 optical defects of, 293 delusions, 350 physics of formation of image in, 282 qualities of visual sensations, 320 reduced schematic (Listing), 286 refractive power of, 293 size of retinal images, 288 spherical aberration in, 294 stereoscopic vision, 347 struggle of visual fields, 344 suppression of visual images, 343 theories of color vision, 331 threshold stimulus of, 317 visual field of, 312 judgments, 349 purple of, 310 Eye-muscles, action of, 338 Facial nerve, dilator fibers in, 545 nucleus of, 229 Far point of distinct vision, 292 Fat, absorption of, 714 as glycogen former, 734 digestion of, 709 in stomach, 699 excessive formation of, in obesity, 799 in bile, 727 metabolism of, in body, 797 nutritive value of, 796 of chyle, 715 origin of, from carbohydrates, 798 in body, 797 Fatigue, in nerve fibers, 110 muscular, 47 of olfactory organs, 279 theories of, 66 Feces, composition of, 720 Fermentation in intestine, 718 of carbohydrates in small intes- tine, 714 Ferments, historical account of, 657 Fertilization of ovum, 850 Fibrillary contractions of heart, 495 Fibrin factors, 417 ferment, preparation of, 416 globulin, 413, 419 relations to blood-clot, 414 Fibrinogen, 412 preparation of, 416 Fictitious meal (Pawlow), 689 Fifth cranial nerve, 227 Fillet, lateral, 200 median, 190 Fistula, Eck's, 754 gall-bladder, 715 stomach, 686 Thiry-Vella, 710 Flavors, 656 dietary importance of, 804 Flechsig's myelinization method, 157 tracts, 164 Fluorid solution, effect on clotting, 422 Food, composition of, 655 definition of, 654 potential energy of, 814 Foodstuffs, definition of, 654 Fourth cranial nerve, nucleus of, 227 Fovea, center for, in occipital cor- tex, 196 of retina, size of, 314, 315 Franklin theory of color vision, 334 Freezing point, method of determin- ing, 884 Fundic glands of stomach, 683 Fundus of stomach, 639 Gall-bladder, functions of, 728 nerves of, 730 Galvanometer, construction of, 92 d'Arsonval, 93 Gases, laws governing absorption of, 599 of blood, 596 pressure of, 599 tension of, in solution, 601 Gas-pump, 597 Gastric glands, 683 histological changes in, during secretion, 684 secretory nerves of, 688 secretion, acid of, 687 composition of, 685 curve of, 689 means of obtaining, 685 normal mechanism of, 689 Gelatin, nutritive value of, 791 Genital organs, vasomotor supply of, 562 Germ plasm, definition of, 872 Globin, 389 Globulicidal action of blood-serum, 387 Globulins, general properties of, 878 Glomerulus of kidney, functions of, 744 Glossopharyngeal nerve, dilator fibers in, 545 894 INDEX. Glossopharyngeal nerve, nucleus of, 229 Glucoproteids, 880 Glucosamin, 875 Glutaminic acid, 706, 874 Glutolin, 411 Glycocholic acid, 725 Glycocoll, 706, 725, 874 Glycogen, amount of, in liver, 732 discovery of, 731 glycogenic theory, 734 importance of, in embryo, 853 in muscle, 59, 736 loss of, during muscular contrac- tions, 64 metabolism of, in body, 794 origin of, 732 supply of, in body, 793 Glycolysis of sugars in body, 782 Glycosuria, 733 alimentary, 714 Glycylglycin, 875 Gmelin's reaction for bile pigments, 724 Golgi's nerve cells, second type, 126 pericellular nerve net, 127 Goll, column of, 162 Gowers's tract, 164 Graafian follicle, structure of, 839 Gram-molecular solution, 883 Growth, 871 Guanase, 664 Guanin, 61, 757 Gudden's commissure, 201 Harmony, physiological cause of, 369 Hearing, cortical center of, 198 limits of, 370 Heart, accelerator center for, 525 nerves of, 522 action current of, 480 of inhibitory nerves, 512 analysis of inhibition, 514 apex beat of, 482 automaticity of, 500 capacity of ventricles of, 489 cardiac nerves of, course, 512 cardiogram, 482 cardio-inhibitory center of, 517 causation of beat, 503 change in form of ventricle in systole, 481 compensatory pause of, 506 contraction wave in, 479 coronary circulation in, 491 course of cardiac nerves, 512 depressor nerve of, 543 diastole and systole of, 477 time relations of, 489 effect of calcium upon, 501 Heart, effect of occlusion of coro- naries upon, 494 of potassium upon, 501 of sodium upon, 501 escape from inhibition, 517 events of a cardiac cycle, 488 fibrillary contractions, 495 inhibition of auricle, 516 of ventricle, 516 intraventricular systole, 484 intrinsic nerves of, 496 maximal contractions of, 504 musculature of, 478 myogenic theory of beat of, 498 negative pressure in, 493 neurogenic theory of beat of, 497 rate of beat as affected by age, 526 by blood-pressure, 527 by extrinsic nerves, 527 by muscular exercise, 528 by sex, 526 by size, 526 by temperature, 529 reflex acceleration of beat, 524 refractory period of, 504 sequence of beat of, 496, 507 sounds of, 485 suction-pump action of, 492 systole and diastole of, 477 time relations of, 489 systolic plateau of beat, 485 theories of inhibition of, 520 tonic inhibition of, 518 tonicity of muscle of, 510 vasomotors of, 550 ventricle of, in systole, 481 work done bv, 489 Heart-block, 508 Heart-muscle, general properties of, 54 Heat centers, 830 equivalent of foodstuffs, 815 loss of, physiological regulation of, 828 nerves, 830 production, physiological regula- tion of, 829 puncture, 832 regulation, general statement of, m 828 rigor of muscle, 51 sexual, in lower animals, 842 Helmholtz theory of color vision, 331 Helweg's bundle, spinal cord, 170 Hematin, 389, 399 Hematoi'din, 724 Hematopoietic tissue, 402 Hematoporphyrin, 400 Hemeralopia (night-blindness), 331 Hemin, 399 Hemipeptone, 705 Hemiplegia, 186 INDEX. 895 Hemochromogen. 389, 400 Hemodromograph, 437 Hemoglobin, absorption spectra of, 394 compounds of, with carbon dioxid, 392 with carbon monoxid, 391 with nitric oxid, 392 with oxygen, 391 condition of, in corpuscles, 385 crystals of, 393 curve of dissociation of oxyhemo- globin, 603 derivative compounds of, 398 iron in, 392 nature and amount of, 389 Hemolysins, natural and acquired, 387 Hemolysis, 385 Hemorrhage, effect of, 425 Heredity, definition and history, 866 Hering theory of color vision, 333 Heterophoria, 340 Hexon bases, 863 Hippuric acid, 759 Histidin, 707, 874 Histohematins, 400 Histons, 863, 879 effect of, on coagulation of blood, 421 Hofacker-Sadler law, 869 Homoiothermous animals, 821 Homolateral conduction in cord, 166 Horopter, 343 Hunger, sense of, 267 Hydrocele liquid, 411 Hydrochloric acid, function of, in pep- tic digestion, 693 in gastric juice, 687 Hydrolysis, 662 Hydrostatic factor in circulation, 465 Hypermetropia, 296 Hyperpnea, 622 Hypertonic solutions, 885 Hypnotic sleep, 248 Hypoglossal nerve, nucleus of, 230 Hypophysis cerebri, 778 Hypotonic solutions, 885 Hypoxanthin, 61, 756 Identical points of retina, 340 Identity theory of nerve impulse, 115 Immune body, 388 Indican, 760 Indirect calorimetry, 827 field of vision, eye, 313 Indol group in proteid molecule, 706 Indoxyl sulphuric acid, 719, 760 Induction coil, 23 Inert layer in circulation, 435 Infundibular body, 778 Inhibition, escape from, in heart, 517 of heart, 513, 527 of knee-jerk, 147 of reflexes, 139 of respiratory movements, 616 reflex of heart, 517 theories of, 520 Inotropic nerves to heart, 516 Inspiration (see also Respiration) definition of, 571 increased heart-rate during, 591 means of producing, 572 muscles of, 574 Inspired air, composition of, 592 Internal secretion, adrenals, 775 definition and historical account, 771 kidney, 782 liver, 771 ovary, 779 pancreas, 780 testis, 778 thyroid tissues, 772 sensations, 256 Intestinal movements, effect of various conditions upon, 648 secretion, 710 Intestines, bacterial action in, 718 large, movements of, 648 nervous control of movements of, 647 reaction of contents of, 718 small, movements of, 644 Intracellular enzymes, 835 Intracranial pressure 556 Intrapulmonic pressure, 583 Intrathoracic pressure, 584 origin of, 586 variations of, with forced breath- ing, 585 Intraventricular pressure, 484 Introductory contractions of muscle, 33 Invertase, 664, 711 in stomach, 697 Involuntary muscle, 52 Iodothyrin, 773 Iris, action of drugs upon, 303 antagonistic action of muscles upon, 304 muscles and nerves of, 299 Iron in hemoglobin, 392 salts, source and nutritive impor- tance of, 803 Irritability, definition of, 22 of muscle, 22 of nerve fibers, 80 Isodynamic equivalent of foodstuffs, 817 Isometric muscular contractions, 27 896 INDEX. Isotonic muscular contractions, 27 solutions, definition of, 885 Isotropous substance in muscle, 19 Jacobson, nerve of, 666 Judgments, visual, of distance and size, 349 of perspective or solidity, 345 Kidney, action of diuretics on, 748 blood-flow through, 749 composition of secretion of (urine), 751 function of convoluted tubules, 746 of glomerulus, 744 internal secretion of, 782 secretion of urine in, 742 structure of, 741 Kjeldahl's method for determination of nitrogen, 752, 784 Knee-jerk, conditions influencing, 149 definition of, 146 explanation of, 148 reinforcement of, 146 use of, as diagnostic sign, 150 Labor, physiology of, 854 Lactic acid in muscle, 60 increase of, during contraction, 64 Laked blood, 385 Langerhans, islands of, 781 Large intestine, digestion and absorp- tion of food in, 717 Larynx, reflex effects from, on respi- rations, 617 Latent period of muscular contrac- tion, 26 Lecithin, 727 Lemniscus, lateral, 200 median, 190 Leucin, 706, 874 Leucocytes, effect of hemorrhage upon, 425 functions of, 405 in formation of thrombin, 417 structure and classification, 404 variations in number of, 405 Leuconuclein, effect upon blood coagulation, 421 Liebermann's reaction for proteids, 877 Liebig's classification of foodstuffs, 809 Light-reflex in eye, 301 Lipase, 662 in pancreatic secretion, 709 in stomach, 697 Lipase, reversible reaction of, 660 Listing's law, 339 schematic eye, 286 Liver, bile-pigments of, 724 acids of, 725 defensive action of, in coagulation, 420 formation of urea in, 737 glycogen of, 731 glycogenic action of, 734 internal secretion of, 771 pulse, 476 quantity of bile, 722 secretion of bile, 727 Localization of function in cerebellum, 224 in cerebrum, 179 Localizing power of skin, 260 Locke's solution, 502 Locomotor ataxia, 163 Ludwig's theory of urinary secretion, 743 Luminosity of visual sensations, 319 Lungs, gaseous exchanges in, 606 vasomotors of, 551 Luxus consumption, 788 Lymph, action of lymphagogues, 430 circulation of, 564 formation of, 427 summary of factors concerned in formation of, 432 Lymphocytes, 404 Lysin, 707, 874 Maltase, 662, 664 Mammary glands, effect of uterus upon, 856 structure and functions, 855 Manometer, Fick's spring, 448 Hiirthle's, 448 maximum and minimum, 449 use of, for determining blood-pres- sure, 443 Mast cells, 404 Mastication, 634 Mathematical perspective, 345 Mean blood-pressure, 446 Medulla oblongata, 225 respiratory center in, 610 Medullary striae, 201 Meningeal spaces, 554 Menstruation, 841 effect of, on other functions, 845 physiological significance of, 845 Mercury manometer, use of, for blood- pressures, 443 Metabolism, definition of, 783 Methemoglobin, 398 Micturition, physiology of, 761 Milk, composition of, 858 Millon's reaction for proteids, 877 INDEX. 897 Minimal air, 580 Miosis, definition of, 303 Molisch reaction for proteids, 877 Monakow's bundle, 169 Motor aphasia, 205 areas of brain,. 183 paths in spinal cordj 167, 169 points in man, 88 Mountain sickness, 629 Movements of alimentary canal, defe- cation, 650 deglutition, 634 large intestine, 648 mastication, 634 small intestine, 644 stomach, 640 vomiting, 651 Mucin in saliva, 669 Muscarin, action of, on heart, 520 on iris, 303 on sweat glands, 768 Muscle, absolute power of, 36 action current of, 96 artificial stimulation of, 23 carbohydrates of, 59 cardiac, properties of, 54 chemical changes of, in contrac- tion, 62 composition of, 57 compound contractions of, 39 contraction of, 26 contraction wave of, 33 contracture of, 33 curve of work of, 36 death rigor, 49 demarcation current of, 91 direct irritability of, 22 disappearance of glycogen in, 63 effect of temperature upon, 28 of veratrin upon, 30 energy liberated in, 34 Engelmann's artificial, 69 enzymes of, 61 ergographic records from, 45 extensibility and elasticity of, 19 fatigue of, 33, 47, 66 glycogen of, 736 heat rigor of, 49 inorganic salts of, 62 introductory contractions of, 33 isotonic and isometric contrac- tions of, 27 lactic acid of, 64 latent period of contraction of, 26 maximal and submaximal contrac- tions of, 27 nitrogenous extractives of, 61 number of stimuli necessary for tetanus, 42 pigments of, 61 plain, 52 plasma, 18, 57 57 Muscle, proteids of, 57 sarcoplasm, 18 simple contraction of, 24 structure of fiber, 18 by polarized light, 19 summation of contractions of, 39 theories of nature of contraction, 68 tone of, during contraction, 41 tonicity of, 48 vasomotor supply of, 563 voluntary contractions of, 43 white and red fibers of, 30 Muscle-sense, cortical area for, 189 distribution and characteristics of, 265 importance of, in visual judg- ments, 346 paths for, in spinal cord, 163, 164, 165 quality of, 266 Muscular insufficiency in movements of eyeballs, 339 work, effect of, on heart rate, 528 on physiological oxidations, 809 on proteid metabolism, 810 on respiratory movements, 627 source of energy in, 811 Musical sounds, classification and properties of, 362 Mydriasis, definition of, 303 Myelin sheath of nerve fibers, func- tion of, 73 Myelinization method of Flechsig, 210 Myogen, 58 fibrin, 58 Myogenic theory of heart beat. 498 Myohematin, 400 Myopia, 295 Myosin, 58 fibrin, 58 Myxedema, 773 Narcosis, effect of, upon nerve im- pulse, 109 Near point of distinct vision, 292 Negative pressure in thorax, 584, 586 in ventricle, 493 variation in muscle and nerve, 96, 98 Nerve, abducens, nucleus of, 229 auditory, 199 chorda tympani, 270, 667 facial, nucleus of, 229 fourth cranial, nucleus of, 227 glossopharyngeal, nucleus of, 229 hypoglossal, nucleus of, 230 motor and sensory roots of, 77 olfactory, 203 898 INDEX. Nerve, optic, 194 recurrent sensibility of anterior roots, 77 spinal accessory, nucleus of, 230 third cranial, nucleus of, 226 trigeminal, nucleus of, 227 twelfth cranial, nucleus of, 230 vagus, nucleus of, 229 Nerve-cell, chromato lysis of, 121 classification of, in spinal cord, 154 degenerative changes in, 120 general physiology of, 128 internal structure of, 126 metabolism in, 129 neuron doctrine, 122 reaction of, 129 refractory period of, 132 summation of stimuli in, 130 varieties of, 124 Nerve-fibers, action current of, 96 afferent and efferent, 75 anodal and cathodal stimula- tion of, 82 antidromic impulses in, 78 artificial stimuli of, 80 classification of, 75 core model of, 10 degeneration and regeneration of, 118 demarcation current of, 91 direction of conduction, 107 du Bois-Reymond's law of stim- ulation, 82 electrical stimulation of, in man^ 89 electrotonic currents of, 101 electrotonus of, 83 impulse in, historical, 104 independent irritability of, 80 metabolism in, during activity, 112 myelin sheath of, 73 nutritive relations to nerve cells, 116 opening and closing tetanus of, 86 Pfliiger's law of stimulation of, 84 stimulation of, in man, 86 structure of, 73 unipolar method of stimulating, 87 Nerve -impulse, historical, 104 metabolism during, 111 modification of, by various in- fluences, 109 qualitative changes in, 76, 115 relation of, to action current, 100, 107 theories of, 113 velocity of, 105 Nervi erigentes, 236, 545 Neurogenic theory of heart beat, 497 Neuron doctrine, 122 Neurons, varieties of, 124 Nicotin, action of, on salivary secre- tion, 676 on sweaty secretion, 768 use of, in tracing autonomic fibers, 233 Night-blindness, hemeralopia, 331 Ninth cranial nerve, nucleus of, 229 Nissl's granules, 127 Nitric oxid hemoglobin, 392 Nitrogen, condition of, in blood, 602 determination of, 784 equilibrium, 783 effect of non-proteid food on, 786 excretion in urine, 752 Nitrogenous extractives of muscle, 61 Nodal point of eye, 286 Non-polarizable electrodes, 95 Normoblasts, 402 Nucleo-albumin, general properties of, 878 in bile, 727 Nucleon, 60 Nucleoproteids, 880 in blood, 414 Obesity, physiological cause of, 799 Ohm's law of composition of sound waves, 365 Olfaction, end-organ for, 275 mechanism of, 276 Olfactometer, 280 Olfactory associations, 281 bulb, histological structure of, 202 center in cortex, 202 nerve, course of fibers of, in brain, 203 organs, fatigue of, 279 sensations, classification of, 278 conflict of, 281 qualities of, 277 threshold stimulus, 280 sense cells, 276 stimuli, nature of, 277 Oncometer, 749 Ophthalmoscope, 305 Opotherapy, 771 Optic chiasma, decussation in, 195 structure of, 194 nerve, course of fibers of, in brain, 193 radiation, 193 thalamus, functions of, 214 in vision, 197 tracts, structure of, 195 Optical deceptions, 350 Optograms on retina, 311 INDEX. 899 Osmosis, definition of, 881 Osmotic pressure, definition of, 882 determination of, 883 of blood, 386 Oval field of Flechsig, 169 Ovaries, internal secretion of, 779 relation of, to menstruation, 843 Overtones, production of, 364 Ovulation, 840, 844 Ovum, fertilization of, 850 implantation of, in uterus, 851 maturation of, 847 passage of, into uterus, 846 Oxalate solutions, effect of, on coagu- lation, 422 Oxidases, 662, 836 Oxidations, theories of, 833 Oxygen, action of, on respiratory center, 620 compound of, with hemoglobin, 391 condition of, in blood, 603 effects of, on muscular irritability, 63 varying pressures of, on respira- tion, 628 tension of, in alveolar air, 607 in tissues, 608 in venous blood, 607 Oxyhemoglobin, 391 Pacchionian bodies of brain, 554 Pain sense, distribution and char- acteristics of, 262 localization of, 263 path for, in cord, 163, 165 Pancreas, action of lipase in, 709 anatomy of, 700 curve of secretion of, 702 digestive action of secretion, 705 on carbohydrates, 705 internal secretion of, 780 normal mechanism of secretion, 703 secretory nerves of, 701 Paraglobulin, 411 Paralysis (motor), 185 Paralytic secretion (saliva), 677 Parapeptone, 693 Parathyroid, structure and function of, 772 Parthenogenesis, 851, 870 Parturition, physiologv of, 854 Pendular movements (intestine), 646 sound waves, 363 Pepsin, 664 discovery of, 657 properties of, 691 Pepsin-hydrochloric acid digestion, 693 Peptids, 708, 875 Peptone, 693, 879 Peptone, absorption of, in stomach, 699 effect on clotting of blood, 423 hemi- and anti-, 705 Peptozym, 423 Pericardial liquid, 410 Perimeter, 312 Peripheral field of vision, 313 resistance in circulation, 462 Peristalsis, 644 Peroxidases, 836 Perspiration (see Sweat) Pettenkofer's reaction, 725 Pfliiger law of stimulation, 84 tetanus, 86 Phagocytes, 405 Phenolsulphuric acid, 719, 760 Phenylalanin, 706, 874 Phloridzin diabetes, 734, 796 Phosphocarnic acid, 60 Phrenology, 180 Physiological diplopia, 342 oxidations, theories of, 820, 833 Lavoisier's work, 569 point (vision), 315 saline, 386, 426 Physostigmin, action of, on iris, 303 Pilocarpin, action of, on heart, 520 on iris, 303 on salivary glands, 676 on sweat glands, 768 Pituitary body, structure and func- tions of, 777 Placenta, functions of, 852 Plain muscle, 52 Plasma of blood, composition of, 408 Plethysmography, 533 Pneumogastric nerve (see Vagus) Pneumograph, 577 Pneumothorax, 587 Poikilothermous animals, 821 Polar bodies of ovum, 848 Polypeptid, 708 Positive electrical variation in heart, 521 Posterior columns, descending tracts of, 169 tracts of, 161 root, termination in cord, 160 cells of, • origin of, 78 Postganglionic nerve fibers, 232 Potassium phosphate in muscle, 65 salts, action of, on heart, 501 Potential energy of food, 814 Precipitins, 876 Predicrotic pulse wave, 474 Preganglionic nerve fibers, 232 Pregnancy, changes in, 854 Prepyramidal tracts, 169 Presbyopia, 292, 297 Pressor nerve fibers, 540 Pressure of gases in solution, 601 900 INDEX. sure sense, distribution of, 256 localizing power, etc., 260 Primary proteoses, 693 Principal axis of lens, 284 focal distance, 283, 286 Projection system of fibers (brain), 173 Propeptone, 693 Prostate gland, 861 Protalbumoses, 693 Protamins, 862, 879 Proteids, absorption of, in intestine, 716 as glycogen-formers, 733 classification of, 877 definition and structure of, 874 diffusion of, 886 excretion of nitrogen of, 752 general reactions of, 875 in blood-plasma, 410, 413 necessary amount of, in diet, 788 normal metabolism in body, 790 nutritive history of, 787 of muscle, 57 osmotic pressure of, 885 putrefaction of, in large intestine, 719 Proteolytic enzymes, 662 Proteoses, general properties of, 879 Prothrombin, 418 Proximate principles of food, 654 Psychophysical law, 253 Ptyalin, 669 action of, in stomach, 697 digestive action of, 679 effect of various conditions upon, 681 Puberty, 841, 860 Pulmonary arteries, vasomotors of, 551 " circulation, peculiarities of, 467 variations of pressure in, 468 Pulse, anacrotic waves of, 475 catacrotic waves of, 474 form of wave, 472 general statement, 469 varieties of, in health and disease, 475 velocity of, propagation of, 470 venous, 475 Pulse-pressure, 446 Purin bases, 61, 739, 756, 863 Purkinje, images of, 290 phenomenon, 319 Putrefaction in intestines, 719 Pyloric glands of stomach, 683 secretion of, 692 Pyramidal tract in brain, 184 in spinal cord, 167 Pyrrol compounds in proteid mole- cule, 706 Pyrrolidin-carbonic acid, 706, 874 Reactions for proteids, 875 Gmelin's, 724 of blood, 382 of contents of small intestine, 718 of urine, 751 Pettenkofer's, 725 Recurrent sensibility of anterior roots. 77 Red blood-corpuscles, chemical com- position of, 409 condition of hemoglobin in, 385 effect of hemorrhage upon, 425 hemolysis of, 385 influence of spleen upon, 738 number and size of, 384 origin and fate of, 401 variations in number of, 402 Reduced hemoglobin, 391 schematic eye, 286 Reflex actions, axon reflexes, 142 classification of, 136 definition and historical, 133 from parts of brain, 142 influence of condition of cord on, 141 of sensory endings upon, 138 inhibition of, 139 of heart through, 517 knee-jerk, 146 of erection and ejaculation, 864 of respiratory center, 612 of vasomotor nerves, 540 preparation of reflex frog, 135 reflex arc, 133 respiratory, from nose, larynx, and pharynx, 617 special and deep reflexes of cord, 151 spinal reflexes, 135, 138 theory of, 137 through peripheral ganglia, 142 through vasodilator nerves, 546 time of, 139 Tiirck's method of studying, 141 Refractive power of eye, 293 Refractory period of heart beat, 504 of nerve cell, 132 Regeneration in nerve fibers, 118 of parts of body, 868 Reinforcement of knee-kick, 146 Rennin, 695 of kidney, 782 Reproduction, changes during preg- nancy, 854 in uterus in menstruation, 842 chemistry of spermatozoa, 862 determination of sex, 868 erection, 863 fertilization of ovum, 850 functions of placenta, 852 general statements, 838 INDEX. 901 Reproduction, growth and senescence, 871 heredity, 866 implantation of ovum, 851 mammary glands, 855 menstruation, 841 nutrition of embryo, 852 ovulation, 840, 844 parturition, 854 properties of spermatozoa, 860 relation of ovaries to menstrua- tion, 843 structure and functions of corpus luteum, 840 of Graafian follicles, 839 Residual air, 580 Resonance, importance of, in func- tions of ear, 366 Respiration, abdominal type, 576 acapnia, 625 accessory centers of, in brain, 619 respiratory movements, 577 anatomy of thorax, 571 apnea, 623 artificial, 581 aspiratory action of, on blood-flow, 587 automatic action of respiratory center, 612 bronchocon stricter and broncho- dilator fibers, 625 calorimeter, 827 capacity of the bronchi, 581 chamber, 785 chemical composition of inspired and expired air, 592 Cheyne-Stokes type of, 632 complemental air, 580 condition of carbon dioxid in blood, 604 costal, 573, 576 definition of inspiration and ex- piration, 571 dissociation of oxyhemoglobin, 603 dyspnea, 622 effect of changes in barometric pressure upon, 629 in composition of air, 627 of muscular work upon, 627 exchange of gases in, 608 forced, 575 functional value of nitrogen, 602 gaseous exchange in lungs, 606 gases of blood. 596 history of, 566 hyperpnea, 622 increased heart-rate during inspira- tion, 591 injurious effect of expired air, 593 intrapulmonic pressure, 583 intrathoracic pressure, 584 mechanism of inspiration, 572 Respiration, methods of recording, 577 minimal air, 580 modified respiratory movementst 633 mountain sickness, 629 muscles of expiration, 574 of inspiration, 574 negative pressure in thorax, 584 normal stimulus of, 619 ventilation of alveoli, 581 of swallowing, 636 origin of negative pressure in thorax, 586 physical theory of, 606 physiological anatomy of organs of, 570 pneumothorax, 587 reflex stimulation of respiratory center, 612 relation of vagus nerve to, 614 residual air, 580 respiratory center, 610, 618 quotient, 631 waves of blood-pressure. 588 secretion of gases in lungs, 608 spinal respiratory centers, 611 supplemental air, 580 tension of gases in alveolar air, 606 in arterial blood, 608 in tissues, 608 in venous blood, 607 tidal air, 580 ventilation, principles of, 595 vital capacity, 579 volumes of air respired, 579 voluntary control of, 618 Respiratory center, 610, 611, 618, 619 automatic activity of, 612 normal stimulus of, 619 reflex stimulation of, 612 quotient, 631 waves of blood-pressure, 588 Retina, action current of, 309 acuity of vision in, 315 after-images from, 323 color blindness of, 325 contrasts in, 324 vision in, 321 corresponding points of, 340 distribution of color sense in, 328 entoptic phenomena, 336 function of rods and cones of, 330 fundamental and complementary colors, 322 light adapted and dark adapted, 318 portion of, stimulated by light, 308 projection ,of, on. occipital lobes, LIBRARY V4/, 902 INDEX. Retina, qualities of visual sensations from, 320 size of fovea in, 314, 315 theories of color vision in, 331 threshold stimulus of, 317 visual purple of, 310 Reversible chemical reactions, 659 Rheoscopic frog preparation, 99 Rhodopsin, 310 Ribs, action of, in inspiration, 573 Rigor of muscle, 49, 51, 62, 66 Ringer's solution, 387 Ritter's tetanus, 86 Rivinus, ducts of, 666 Rods and cones of eye, functions of, 330 Sacculus, functions of, 379 Saliva, chorda, 673 composition of, 669 digestive action of, 679 general functions of, 681 secretory nerves for, 670 sympathetic, 673 Sahvarv glands, action of drugs upon, 676 anatomical relations, 666 histological changes during ac- tivity, 674 structure of, 668 normal stimulation of, 678 secretory center for, 679 nerve fibers of, 672 theory of secretory nerves, 673 Salts, absorption of, in stomach, 698 excretion of, 760 general nutritive importance of, 801 Sarco lactic acid, 60 Sebaceous glands, secretion of, 769 Secondary axes of a lens, 284 degeneration, nerve fibers, 117, 157 proteoses, 693 Secretion, 703, 711 internal, 771 of bile, 727 of sebaceous glands, 769 of sweat, 767 of urine, 742 Secretogogues of gastric glands, 690 Secretory nerves of salivary glands, 671 Semicircular canals, direct stimula- tion of, 374 Flourens's experiments upon, 372 • structure of, 372 temporary and permanent effects of operations on, 374 theories of functions of, 375 Seminal vesicles, function of, 861 Senescence, 871 Senses, classification of, 249 cutaneous, 256 Sensory aphasia, 206 areas of cortex, 188 paths in spinal cord, 161 Sequence of heart beat, 507 Serin, 874 Serum, action of foreign, 389 -albumin, 410 -globulin, 411 Seventh cranial nerve, nucleus of. 229 Sex, determination of, 868 Side-pressure in blood-vessels, 460 Sixth cranial nerve, nucleus of, 229 Skatoxylsulphuric acid, 719, 760 Skeletal muscular tissue, physiology of, 18 Skin, excretory functions of, 766 sweat glands of, 766 Sleep, changes in circulation during, 242 curves of intensity of, 239 effect of sensory stimulation dur- ing, 243 hypnotic, 248 metabolism during, 811 physiological phenomena of, 238 theories of, 244 Small intestine, absorption in, 712 Smegma prseputii, 770 Smell, center for, in brain, 204 end-organ of, 275 Smelling, mechanism of, 276 Sodium chlorid, nutritive value of, 802 salts, effect of, on heart-beat, 501 Somatoplasm, definition of, 872 Sound, sensations of (see Ear) waves, nature and action of, 361 overtones of, 364 simple and compound, 363 Specific energy of taste sensations, 273 gravity of blood, 383 nerve energies, doctrine of, 115, 251 of cutaneous nerves, 258 Spectra, absorption, blood, 394 Spectroscope, 395 Spermatozoa, chemistry of, 862 maturation of, 861 properties of, 860 Spermin, 779 Spherical aberration in eye, 294 Sphygmography, 472 Sphygmomanometers for determin- ing blood-pressure in man, 456 Spinal cord, anterior marginal bundle of, 170 as path of conduction, 154 classification of tracts in, 157 INDEX. 903 Spinal cord, effect of removing, 144, 651 general relations of gray and white matter in, 156 groups of cells in, 154 Helweg's bundle in, 170 homolateral and contralateral conduction in, 166 less well-known tracts of, 169 Monakow's bundle in, 169 paths in, for cutaneous im- pulses, 165 pyramidal tracts of, 167 reflex activities of, 133, 144 tonic activity of, 144 tracts of, in lateral column, 164 in posterior column, 164 in white matter, 156 ventral longitudinal bundle, 170 vestibulo-spinal tract, 170 reflex movements, 135 reflexes in mammals, 138 respiratory center, 611 Spirometer, 579 Spleen, physiology of, 738 Stannius, first ligature of, 499 Stapedius muscle, function of, 358 Starvation, effect of, on body-metab- olism, 812 Steapsin (see Lipase) Stenson's duct, 666 Stereognostic sense, 189 Stereoscopic vision, 347 Stethoscope, 485 Stimulants, 656 nutritive importance of, 805 Stimuli, artificial, of muscle, 23 Stokes- Adams syndrome, 508 Stokes's reducing agent, 397 Stomach, absorption in, 698 anatomy of, 639 automaticity of, 643 digestion in, 696 glands of, 683 histological changes in, during activity, 684 means of obtaining secretion of, 685 mechanism of gastric secretion, 689 movements of, 640 musculature of, 640 properties of pepsin of, 691 secretory nerves of, 688 Strabismus, 340 Stromuhr, 436 Submaxillary ganglion, 668 Succus entericus, 710 Sugars, absorption of, in stomach, 699 metabolism of, in body, 794 Sulphur, forms in which excreted, 759 Summation of muscular contractions, 39 of stimuli in nerve centers, 130 Superior olivary body, relation of, to auditory paths, 200 Supplemental air, 580 Suprarenalin, 776 Swallowing, 634 respiration, 636 Sweat, amount and composition of, 766 nerve centers for, 769 secretory fibers of, 767 Sweat-nerves, action of, in heat regu- lation, 829 Sympathetic nervous system, general relations, 231 resonance, 366 Syntonin, 693 Systole, duration of, in heart. 489 Systolic arterial pressure, 446 determination of, in animals, 447 in man, 453 plateau of ventricular contraction, 485 Tabes dorsalis, 163 Taste buds, 275 center for, in brain , 204 nerves of, 270 sensations, classification of, 272 specific energy of, 273 threshold stimulus, 275 sense of, 270 stimuli, mode of action, 274 Taurin, 725 Taurocholic acid, 725 Tectorial membrane, 368 Temperature (see Heat) effect of, on bo&y metabolism, 811 on gases in solution, 600 on heart rate, 529 on muscular contraction, 28 of human body, 822 sense, distribution and character- istics, 259 path for, in spinal cord, 163, 165 punctiform distribution of, 257 Tension of gases in solution, 601 Tensor tympani muscle, function of, 358 Tenth cranial nerve, nucleus of, 229 Testis, internal secretion of, 778 Tetanus, number of stimuli necessary for, 42 of artificial muscle, 71 of muscle, 39 opening and closing, 86 Theobromin, 757, 805 Third cranial nerve, nucleus of, 226 904 INDEX. Thirst, sense of, 267 Thorax, as closed cavity, 571 aspiratory action of, 587 normal position of, 571 origin of negative pressure in, 586 Thrombin, 416 Thrombokinase, 418 Thyroid, extirpation of, 772 function of, 774 internal secretion of, 772 theory of (Cyon), 775 Tidal air, 580 Tigroid substance in nerve cells, 127 Tissue proteid, 787 Tissues, exchange of gases in, 608 Tone (musical) of muscular con- traction, 41 Tonicity of heart muscle, 510 of muscle, 48 of nerve centers, 144 Touch sense, path of, in cord, 169 Tracts in spinal cord, 157, 169 of Burdach 159 of Flechsig, 159, 164 of Goll, 159 of Gowers, 159, 164 Traube-Hering waves, 544 Treppe of muscular contractions, 33 Trigeminal nerve, area of distribu- tion of, 228 nucleus of, 227 Trophic nerve fibers, salivary glands, 672 Trypsin, 664, 707 Tryptophan, 707, 874 Tiirck's method for reflex time, 141 Twelfth cranial nerve, nucleus of, 230 Tympanic membrane, 354 Tyrosin, 706, 874 Unipolar method of stimulation, 87 Urea, formation of, in liver, 737 origin and significance of, 753 Ureters, contractions of, 761 Urethra, sphincter of, 763 Uric acid, 61 in spleen, 739 significance of, 757 Urinary bladder, movements of, 762 nerves of, 765 pigments, 400 Urination, physiological mechanism of, 761 Urine, composition of, 751 nitrogenous excreta of, 752 origin of acidity of, 748 osmotic pressure of, 746 reaction of. 751 secretion of, 742 secretion pressure of, 745 Uterus, changes of, during menstrua- tion, 842 effect of, on mammary glands, 856 Utriculus, function of, 379 Vagus nerve, action of, on gastric secretion, 688 on heart, 516 (see also In- hibition) on pancreas, 701 as motor nerve to stomach, 643 nucleus of, 229 relations of, to respiratory cen- ter, 614 Vasomotor fibers, centers for, in cord, 543 in brain, 552 to pulmonary arteries, 551 nerves, action of, in heat regula- tion, 829 antidromic impulses in, 548 course and distribution, 535, 545 general properties of dilators, 545 history of discovery of, 531 methods used to demonstrate,. 532 of heart, 550 position of constrictor center, 538 reflex actions of, 540 rhythmical action of constric- tor center, 544 to abdominal organs, 562 to dilator center and reflexes, 546 to genital organs, 562 to head, 561 to kidneys, 749 to skeletal muscles, 563 to trunk and limbs, 562 to veins, 564 tonic activity of, 538 Veins, vasomotor supply of, 564 Velocity of blood-flow (see Circu- lation) pressure in blood-vessels, 460 Venous pulse, 475 in brain sinuses, 558 Ventilation, principles of, 595 Ventral longitudinal bundle in spinal cord, 170 Ventricle (see Heart) Veratrin, effect of, on heart, 30 Vernix caseosa, 770 Vestibulo-spinal tract of spinal cord,, 170 Vision (see Eye and Retina) Visual acuity, 315 center in cortex, 192 INDEX. 905 Visual field, binocular, 340 monocular, 312 fields, conflict of, in binocular vision, 344 purple, 310 Vital capacity, 579 Voluntary muscular contractions, 43 Vomiting, 651 Wallerian degeneration, 117, 157 Warm spots of skin, 257 Water, absorption of, in stomach, 698 excretion of, 760 Weber-Fechner law, 253 Wharton's duct, 666 Wirsung, duct of, 700 Work done by contracting muscle, 37 Xanthin, 61, 756 Xanthoproteic reaction for proteids, 877 Zymogen, 663 granules, 675 Zymoplastic substances in coagula- tion of blood, 418 SAUNDERS* BOOKS on Skin, Genito-Urinary Diseases, Chemistry, and Eye, Ear, Nose, and Throat W. B. SAUNDERS COMPANY 925 WALNUT STREET PHILADELPHIA 9, HENRIETTA STREET, COVENT GARDEN, LONDON MECHANICAL EXCELLENCE |kJOT alone for their literary excellence have the Saunders publi- * ™ cations become a standard on both sides of the Atlantic : their mechanical perfection is as universally commended as is their sci- entific superiority. The most painstaking attention is bestowed upon all the details that enter into the mechanical production of a book, and medical journals, both at home and abroad, in reviewing the Saunders publications, seldom fail to speak of this distinguishing feature. The attainment of this perfection is due to the fact that the firm has its own Art Department, in which photographs and drawings of a very high order of merit are produced. This department is of decided value to authors, in enabling them to procure the services of artists specially skilled in the various methods of illustrating medical publications. A Complete Catalogue of our Publications will be Sent upon Request SAUNDERS' BOOKS ON StelwagonV Diseases of the Skin A Treatise on Diseases of the Skin. For Advanced Students and Practitioners. By Henry W. Stelwagon, M. D., Ph. D., Professor of Dermatology in the Jefferson Medical College, and Clinical Professor of Dermatology in the Woman's Medical College, Philadelphia. Hand- some octavo volume of 1135 pages, with 258 text-cuts and 32 full- page colored lithographic and half-tone plates. Cloth, $6.00 net; Sheep or Half Morocco, $7.00 net. JUST ISSUED— NEW (4th) EDITION, REVISED FOUR LARGE EDITIONS IN THREE YEARS The demand for four editions of this work in a period of three years, and the many gratifying review notices indicate beyond a doubt the practical character of the book. In preparing the work the predominant aim kept in view was to sup- ply the physician with a treatise written on plain and practical lines, giving abun- dant helpful case illustrations. In this edition, although some fifty new illustra- tions have been added, the size of the work has not been increased, many old illustrations having been eliminated and the text, wherever possible, made more concise. PERSONAL AND PRESS OPINIONS John T. Bowen, M.D., Assistant Professor of Dermatology, Harvard University Medical School, Boston. " It gives me great pleasure to endorse Dr. Stelwagon's book. The clearness of description is a marked feature. It is also very carefully compiled. It is one of the best text-books yet published and a credit to American dermatology." Grover W. Wende, M.D., Clinical Professor of Dermatology, University of Buffalo. " I have recommended that the work be included in the catalogue of the University of Buffalo. I think you are to be congratulated on bringing out one of the very best works published in the English language." Boston Medical and Surgical Journal " We can cordially recommend Dr. Stelwagon's book to the profession as the best text- book on dermatology, for the advanced student and general practitioner, that has been brought Strictly up to date. . . . The photographic illustrations are numerous, and many of them are of great excellence." DISEASES OF THE EYE. DeSchweinitz's Diseases of the Eye Just Issued— The New (5th) Edition, Enlarged Diseases of the Eye : A Handbook of Ophthalmic Practice. By G. E. DeSchweinitz, M.D., Professor of Ophthalmology in the Uni- versity of Pennsylvania, Philadelphia, etc. Handsome octavo of 894 pages, 313 text-illustrations, and 6 chromo-lithographic plates. Cloth, $5.00 net; Sheep or Half Morocco, $6.00 net. WITH 313 TEXT-ILLUSTRATIONS AND 6 COLORED PLATES For this new edition the text has been very thoroughly revised, and the work enlarged by the addition of new matter to the extent of some one hundred pages. There have been added, amongst other subjects, chapters on the following : X-Ray Treatment of Epithelioma, Xeroderma Pigmentosum ; Purulent Conjunctivitis of Young Girls ; Jequiritol and Jequiritol Serum ; X-ray Treatment of Trachoma ; Infected Marginal Ulcer ; Keratitis Punctata Syphilitica ; Uveitis and Its Varieties ; Eye- ground Lesions of Hereditary Syphilis ; Macular Atrophy of the Retina; Worth's Amblyoscope ; Stovain, Alypin ; Motais' Operation for Ptosis ; Kuhnt- Miiller's Operation for Ectropion ; Haab's Method for Foreign Bodies ; and Sweet's X-Ray Method of Localizing Foreign Bodies. Other chapters have been rewritten. The excellence of the illustrative feature has been maintained. PERSONAL AND PRESS OPINIONS Samuel Theobald, M.D., Clinical Professor of Ophthalmology, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore. " It is a work that I have held in high esteem, and is one of the two or three books upon the eye which I have been in the habit of recommending to my students in the Johns Hopkins Medical School." Late William Pepper, M.D., Professor of Theory and Practice of Medicine and Clinical Medicine, University of Penn- sylvania. "A work that will meet the requirements not only of the specialist, but also of the general practitioner in a rare degree. I am satisfied that unusual success awaits it." British Medical Journal " A clearly written, comprehensive manual. One which we can commend to students as a reliable text-book, written with an evident knowledge of the wants of those entering upon the study of this special branch of medical science." SAUNDERS' BOOKS ON Barton and Well./*' Medical Thesaurus A NEW WORK— RECENTLY ISSUED A Thesaurus of Medical Words and Phrases. By Wilfred M. Barton, A. M., Assistant to Professor of Materia Medica and Thera- peutics, and Lecturer on Pharmacy, Georgetown University, Washing- ton, D. C. ; and Walter A. Wells, M. D., Demonstrator of Laryn- gology, Georgetown University, Washington, D. C. Handsome i2mo of 534 pages. Flexible leather, $2.50 net; with thumb index, $3.00 net. THE ONLY MEDICAL THESAURUS EVER PUBLISHED This work is unique in that it is the only Medical Thesaurus ever published. Instead of supplying the meaning to given words, as an ordinary dictionary does, it reverses the process, and when the meaning or idea is in the mind it endeavors to supply the fitting term or phrase to express that idea. This Thesaurus will be of service to all persons who are called upon to state or explain any subject in the technical language of medicine. Boston Medical and Surgical Journal " We can easily see the value of such a book, and can certainly recommend it to our readers." Saxe's Urinalysis Examination of the Urine. By G. A. De Santos Saxe, M. D., Pathologist to Columbus Hospital, New York City. i2mo of 391 pages, fully illustrated. Flexible leather, $1.50 net. RECENTLY ISSUED This work is intended as an aid in diagnosis, by interpreting the clinical significance of the chemic and microscopic urinary findings. The author has introduced some of his own methods of working. EYE, EAR, NOSE, AND THROAT. American Text-Book qf Eye, Ear, Nose, and Throat American Text-Book of Diseases of the Eye, Ear, Nose, and Throat. Edited by G. E. de Schweinitz, M.D., Professor of Ophthal- mology in the University of Pennsylvania ; and B. Alexander Randall, M. D., Clinical Professor of Diseases of the Ear in the University of Pennsylvania. Imperial octavo, 125 1 pages, with y66 illustrations, 59 of them in colors. Cloth, $7.00 net ; Sheep or Half Morocco, $8.00 net. This work is essentially a text-book on the one hand, and, on the other, a volume of reference to which the practitioner may turn and find a series of articles written by representative authorities on the subjects portrayed by them. There- fore, the practical side of the question has been brought into prominence. Par- ticular emphasis has been laid on the most approved methods of treatment. American Journal of the Medical Sciences " The different articles are complete, forceful, and, if one may be permitted to use the term, 'snappy,' in decided contrast to some of the labored but not more learned descriptions which have appeared in the larger systems of ophthalmology." Hyde and Montgomery's Syphilis and Venereal Syphilis and the Venereal Diseases. By James Nevins Hyde, M. D., Professor of Skin, Genito-Urinary, and Venereal Diseases, and Frank H. Montgomery, M. D., Associate Professor of Skin, Genito- Urinary, and Venereal Diseases in Rush Medical College, in Affiliation with the University of Chicago, Chicago. Octavo volume of 594 pages, profusely illustrated. Cloth, $4.00 net. SECOND EDITION, REVISED AND GREATLY ENLARGED In this edition every page has received careful revision ; many subjects, notably that on Gonorrhea, have been practically rewritten, and much new mate- rial has been added. A number of new cuts have also been introduced, besides a series of beautiful colored lithographic plates. American Journal of Cutaneous and Genito-Urinary Diseases " It is a plain, practical, and up-to-date manual containing just the kind of information that physicians need to cope successfully with a troublesome class of diseases." SAUNDERS' BOOKS ON GET A ^„ A<#2 — ^ -^ THE NEW THE BEST /\IilCrlViC\ri STANDARD Illustrated Dictionary Third Revised Edition— Recently Issued The American Illustrated Medical Dictionary. A new and com- plete dictionary of the terms used in Medicine, Surgery, Dentistry, Pharmacy, Chemistry, and kindred branches ; with over ioo new and elaborate tables and many handsome illustrations. By W. A. Newman Dorland, M. D., Editor of " The American Pocket Medical Diction- ary." Large octavo, nearly 800 pages, bound in full flexible leather. Price, $4.50 net ; with thumb index, $5.00 net. THREE EDITIONS IN THREE YEARS-WITH 1500 NEW TERMS In this edition the book has been subjected to a thorough revision. The author has also added upward of fifteen hundred important new terms that have appeared in medical literature during the past few months. Howard A. Kelly, M. D., Professor of Gynecology, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore. "Dr. Dorland's Dictionary is admirable. It is so well gotten up and of such convenient size. No errors have been found in my use of it." American Year-Book Saunders' American Year-Book of Medicine and Surgery. A Yearly Digest of Scientific Progress and Authoritative Opinion in all Branches of Medicine and Surgery, drawn from journals, monographs, and text-books of the leading American and foreign authors and inves- tigators. Arranged, with critical editorial comments, by eminent American specialists, under the editorial charge of George M. Gould, A. M., M. D. In two volumes : Vol. I. — General Medicine, octavo, 715 pages, illustrated ; Vol. II. — General Surgery, octavo, 684 pages, illus- trated. Per vol. : Cloth, $3.00 net ; Half Morocco, $3.75 net. Sold by Subscription. In these volumes the reader obtains not only a yearly digest, but also the invaluable annotations and criticisms of the editors. As usual, this issue of the Year-Book is amply illustrated. The Lancet, London " It is much more than a mere compilation of abstracts, for, as each section is entrusted to experienced and able contributors, the reader has the advantage of certain critical commen- taries and expositions . . . proceeding from writers fully qualified to perform these tasks." NOSE, THROAT, AND EAR. Gradle's Nose, Pharynx, and Ear Diseases of the Nose, Pharynx, and Ear. By Henry Gradle, M. D., Professor of Ophthalmology and Otology, Northwestern Uni- versity Medical School, Chicago. Handsome octavo of 547 pages, illustrated, including two full-page plates in colors. Cloth, $3.50 net. INCLUDING TOPOGRAPHIC ANATOMY This volume presents diseases of the Nose, Pharynx, and Ear as the author has seen them during an experience of nearly twenty-five years. In it are answered in detail those questions regarding the course and outcome of diseases which cause the less experienced observer the most anxiety in an individual case. Topographic anatomy has been accorded liberal space. Pennsylvania Medical Journal "This is the most practical volume on the nose, pharynx, and ear that has appeared recently. ... It is exactly what the less experienced observer needs, as it avoids the confusion incident to a categorical statement of everybody's opinion." Kyle's Diseases of Nose and Throat Diseases of the Nose and Throat. By D. Braden Kyle, M. D., Professor of Laryngology in the Jefferson Medical College, Phila- delphia; Consulting Laryngologist, Rhinologist, and Otologist, St. Agnes' Hospital. Octavo, 669 pages; over 184 illustrations, and 26 lithographic plates in colors. Cloth, $4.00 net. RECENTLY ISSUED— THIRD REVISED EDITION Three large editions of this excellent work fully testify to its practical value. In this edition the author has revised the text thoroughly, bringing it absolutely down to date. With the practical purpose of the book in mind, ex- tended consideration has been given to treatment, each disease being considered in full, and definite courses being laid down to meet special conditions and symptoms. Dudley S. Reynolds, M. D., Formerly Professor of Ophthalmology and Otology, Hospital College of Medicine, Louisville. " It is an important addition to the text-books now in use, and is better adapted to the uses of the student than any other work with which I am familiar. I shall be pleased to commend Dr. Kyle's work as the best text-book." SAUNDERS' BOOKS ON Briihl, Politzer, and Smith's Otology Atlas and Epitome of Otology. By Gustav Bruhl, M. D., of Berlin, with the collaboration of Professor Dr. A. Politzer, of Vienna. Edited, with additions, by S. MacCuen Smith, M.D., Pro- fessor of Otology in the Jefferson Medical College, Philadelphia. With 244 colored figures on 39 lithographic plates, 99 text illustra- tions, and 292 pages of text. Cloth, $3.00 net. In Saunders1 Hand- Atlas Series. INCLUDING ANATOMY AND PHYSIOLOGY The work is both didactic and clinical in its teaching. A special feature is the very complete exposition of the minute anatomy of the ear, a working knowl- edge of which is so essential to an intelligent conception of the science of otology. The association of Professor Politzer and the use of so many valuable specimens from his notably rich collection especially enhance the value of the treatise. The work contains everything of importance in the elementary study of otology. Clarence J. Blake, M. D., Professor of Otology in Harvard University Medical School, Boston. " The most complete work of its kind as yet published, and one commending itself to both the student and the teacher in the character and scope of its illustrations." Haab and deSchweinitz's Operative Ophthalmology Atlas and Epitome of Operative Ophthalmology. By Dr. O. Haab, of Zurich. Edited, with additions, by G. E. de Schweinitz, M. D., Professor of Ophthalmology in the University of Pennsylvania. With 30 colored lithographic plates, 1 54 text-cuts, and 375 pages of text. In Saunders' Hand- Atlas Series. Cloth, $3.50 net. RECENTLY ISSUED Dr. Haab's Atlas of Operative Ophthalmology will be found as beautiful and as practical as his two former atlases. The work represents the author' s thirty years' experience in eye work. The various operative interventions are described with all the precision and clearness that such an experience brings. Recognizing the fact that mere verbal descriptions are frequently insufficient to give a clear idea of operative procedures, Dr. Haab has taken particular care to illustrate plainly the different parts of the operations. Johns Hopkins Hospital Bulletin " The descriptions of the various operations are so clear and full that the volume can well hold place with more pretentious text-books." DISEASES OF THE EYE. Haab and DeSchweinitz's External Diseases qf the Eye Atlas and Epitome of External Diseases of the Eye. By Dr. O. Haab, of Zurich. Edited, with additions, by G. E. deSchweinitz, M. D., Professor of Ophthalmology, University of Pennsylvania. With 98 colored illustrations on 48 lithographic plates and 232 pages of text. Cloth, $3.00 net. In Saunders' Hand-Atlas Series. SECOND REVISED EDITION— RECENTLY ISSUED Conditions attending diseases of the external eye, which are often so complicated, have probably never been more clearly and comprehensively expounded than in the forelying work, in which the pictorial most happily supplements the verbal description. The price of the book is remarkably low. The Medical Record, New York " The work is excellently suited to the student of ophthalmology and to the practising physician. It cannot fail to attain a well-deserved popularity." Haab and DeSchweinitzV Ophthalmoscopy Atlas and Epitome of Ophthalmoscopy and Ophthalmoscopic Diagnosis. By Dr. O. Haab, of Zurich. From the Third Revised and Enlarged German Edition. Edited, with additions, by G. E. deSchweinitz, M. D., Professor of Ophthalmology, University of Pennsylvania. With 152 colored lithographic illustrations and 85 pages of text. Cloth, $3.00 net. In Saunders' Hand- Atlas Series. The great value of Prof. Haab's Atlas of Ophthalmoscopy and Ophthalmo- scopic Diagnosis has been fully established and entirely justified an English translation. Not only is the student made acquainted with carefully prepared ophthalmoscopic drawings done into well-executed lithographs of the most im- portant fundus changes, but, in many instances, plates of the microscopic lesions are added. The whole furnishes a manual of the greatest possible service. The Lancet, London "We recommend it as a work that should be in the ophthalmic wards or in the library of every hospital into which ophthalmic cases are received." io SAUNDERS' BOOKS ON Grunwald and Grayson's Diseases of the Larynx Atlas and Epitome of Diseases of the Larynx. By Dr. L. Grun- wald, of Munich. Edited, with additions, by Charles P. Grayson, M. D., Clinical Professor of Laryngology and Rhinology, University of Pennsylvania. With 107 colored figures on 44 plates, 25 text-cuts, and 103 pages of text. Cloth, $2.50 net. In Saunders' Hand-Atlas Series. British Medical Journal " Excels everything we have hitherto seen in the way of colored illustrations of diseases of the larynx. . . . Not only valuable for the teaching of laryngology, it will prove of the greatest help to those who are perfecting themselves by private study." American Text-Book of Genito-Urinary, Syphilis, Skin American Text-book of Genito=Urinary Diseases, Syphilis, and Diseases of the Skin. Edited by L. Bolton Bangs, M. D., late Prof, of Genito-Urinary Surgery, University and Bellevue Hospital Medical College, New York; and W. A. Hardaway, M. D., Professor of Diseases of the Skin, Missouri Medical College. Imperial octavo, 1229 pages, with 300 engravings, 20 colored plates. Cloth, $7.00 net Journal of the American Medical Association " This voluminous work is thoroughly up-to-date, and the chapters on genito-urinary dis- eases are especially valuable. The illustrations are fine and are mostly original. The section on dermatology is concise and in every way admirable." SennV Genito-Urinary Tuberculosis Tuberculosis of the Genito-Urinary Organs, Male and Female. By N. Senn, M. D., Ph. D., LL.D., Professor of Surgery in Rush Med- ical College. Octavo of 317 pages, illustrated. Cloth, $3.00 net. British Medical Journal " The book will well repay perusal. It is the final word, as our knowledge stands, upon the diseases of which it treats, and will add to the reputation of its distinguished author." DISEASES OF THE SKIN. Mracek and Stelwagon's Diseases of the Skin Atlas and Epitome of Diseases of the Skin. By Prof. Dr. Franz Mracek, of Vienna. Edited, with additions, by Henry W. Stelwagon, M. D., Professor of Dermatology in the Jefferson Medical College, Philadelphia. With JJ colored plates, 50 half-tone illustrations, and 280 pages of text. In Saunders^ Hand-Atlas Series. Go., $4. 00 net. JUST ISSUED— NEW (2d) EDITION This volume, the outcome of years of scientific and artistic work, contains, together with colored plates of unusual beauty, numerous illustrations in black, and a text comprehending the entire field of dermatology. The illustrations are all original and prepared from actual cases in Mracek' s clinic, and the execution of the plates is superior to that of any, even the most expensive, dermatologic atlas hitherto published. American Journal of the Medical Sciences " The advantages which we see in this book and which recommend it to our minds are: First, its handiness ; secondly, the plates, which are excellent as regards drawing, color, and the diagnostic points which they bring out." Mracek and Bangs' Syphilis and Venereal Atlas and Epitome of Syphilis and the Venereal Diseases. By Prof. Dr. Franz Mracek, of Vienna. Edited, with additions, by L. Bolton Bangs, M. D., late Prof, of Genito-Urinary Surgery, Univer- sity and Bellevue Hospital Medical College, New York. With 71 colored plates and 122 pages of text. Cloth, $3.50 net. In Saunders' Hand- Atlas Series. CONTAINING 71 COLORED PLATES According to the unanimous opinion of numerous authorities, to whom the original illustrations of this book were presented, they surpass in beauty anything of the kind that has been produced in this field, not only in Germany, but throughout the literature of the world. Robert L. Dickinson, M. D. . Art Editor of " The American Text-Book of Obstetrics." " The book that appeals instantly to me for the strikingly successful, valuable, and graphic character of its illustrations is the ' Atlas of Syphilis and the Venereal Diseases.' I know of nothing in this country that can compare with it." 12 SAUNDERS' BOOKS ON Holland's Medical Chemistry and Toxicology A Text-Book of Medical Chemistry and Toxicology. By James W. Holland, M.D., Professor of Medical Chemistry and Toxicology, and Dean, Jefferson Medical College, Philadelphia. Octavo of 592 pages, fully illustrated. Cloth, $3.00 net. RECENTLY ISSUED Dr. Holland's work is an entirely new one, and is based on his thirty-five years' practical experience in teaching chemistry and medicine. Recognizing that to understand physiologic chemistry, students must first be informed upon points not referred to in most medical text-books, the author has included in his work the latest views of equilibrium of equations, mass action, cryoscopy, os- motic pressure, (Association of salts into ions, effects of ionization upon electric conductivity, and the relationship between purin bodies, uric acid, and urea. More space is given to toxicology than in any other text-book on chemistry. American Medicine " Its statements are clear and terse; its illustrations well chosen ; its development logical, systematic, and comparatively easy to follow. . . . We heartily commend the work." Grtinwald and Newcomb's Mouth, Pharynx, and Nose Atlas and Epitome of Diseases of the Mouth, Pharynx, and Nose. By Dr. L. Grunwald, of Munich. From the Second Revised and Enlarged German Edition. Edited, with additions, by James E. Newcomb, M. D., Instructor in Laryngology, Cornell University Medical School. With 102 illustrations on 42 colored lithographic plates, 41 text-cuts, and 219 pages of text. Cloth, $3.00 net. In Saunders' Hand-Atlas Series. INCLUDING ANATOMY AND PHYSIOLOGY In designing this atlas the needs of both student and practitioner were kept constantly in mind, and as far as possible typical cases of the various diseases were selected. The illustrations are described in the text in exactly the same way as a practised examiner would demonstrate the objective findings to his class. The illustrations themselves are numerous and exceedingly well executed. The editor has incorporated his own valuable experience, and has also included exten- sive notes on the use of the active principle of the suprarenal bodies. American Medicine " Its conciseness without sacrifice of clearness and thoroughness, as well as the excellence of text and illustrations, are commendable." EYE, EAR, NOSE, AND THROAT. 13 Jackson on the Eye A Manual of the Diagnosis and Treatment of Diseases of the Eye. By Edward Jackson, A. M., M. D., Emeritus Professor of Diseases of the Eye in the Philadelphia Polyclinic. i2mo volume of 535 pages, with 178 beautiful illustrations, mostly from drawings by the author. Cloth, $2.50 net. The Medical Record, New York " It is truly an admirable work. . . . Written in a clear, concise manner, it bears evidence of the author's comprehensive grasp of the subject. The term ' multum in parvo ' is an appro- priate one to apply to this work. It will prove of value to all who are interested in this branch of medicine." Grant on the Face, Mouth, and Jaws A Text- Book of the Surgical Principles and Surgical Diseases of the Face, Mouth, and Jaws. For Dental Students. By H. Horace Grant, A. M., M. D., Professor of Surgery and of Clinical Surgery, Hospital College of Medicine, Louisville. Octavo of 231 pages, with 68 illustrations. Cloth, $2.50 net. Annals of Surgery " The book is well illustrated, the text is clear, and on the whole it serves well for the purpose for which it is intended." Friedrich and Curtis' Nose, Larynx, and Ear Rhinology, Laryngology, and Otology, and Their Significance in General Medicine. By Dr. E. P. Friedrich, of Leipzig. Edited by H. Holbrook Curtis, M.D., Consulting Surgeon to the New York Nose and Throat Hospital. Octavo volume of 350 pages. Cloth, #2.50 net. Boston Medical and Surgical Journal " This task he has performed admirably, and has given both to the general practitioner and to the specialist a book for collateral reference which is modern, clear, and complete." 14 SAUNDERS' BOOKS ON Og'den on the Urine Clinical Examination of Urine and Urinary Diagnosis. A Clinical Guide for the Use of Practitioners and Students of Medicine and Sur- gery. By J. Bergen Ogden, M. D., Late Instructor in Chemistry, Harvard University Medical School; Formerly Assistant in Clinical Pathology, Boston City Hospital. Octavo, 418 pages, 54 illustrations, and a number of colored plates. Cloth, $3.00 net. SECOND REVISED EDITION— RECENTLY ISSUED In this edition the work has been brought absolutely down to the present day. Important changes have been made in connection with the determination of Urea, Uric Acid, and Total Nitrogen ; and the subjects of Cryoscopy and Beta-Oxybutyric Acid have been given a place. Special attention has been paid to diagnosis by the character of the urine, the diagnosis of diseases of the kidneys and urinary passages ; an enumeration of the prominent clinical symptoms of each disease ; and the peculiarities of the urine in certain general diseases. The Lancet, London " We consider this manual to have been well compiled ; and the author's own experience, so clearly stated, renders the volume a useful one both for study and reference." Vecki's Sexual Impotence The Pathology and Treatment of Sexual Impotence. By Victor G. Vecki, M. D. From the Second Revised and Enlarged German Edition. i2mo volume of 329 pages. Cloth, $2.00 net. THIRD EDITION, REVISED AND ENLARGED The subject of impotence has but seldom been treated in this country in the truly scientific spirit that its pre-eminent importance deserves, and this volume will come to many as a revelation of the possibilities of therapeutics in this important field. The reading part of the English-speaking medical profession has passed judgment on this monograph. The whole subject of sexual impotence and its treatment is discussed by the author in an exhaustive and thoroughly scientific manner. In this edition the book has been thoroughly revised, and new matter has been added, especially to the portion dealing with treatment. Johns Hopkins Hospital Bulletin " A scientific treatise upon an important and much neglected subject. . . . The treatment of impotence in general and of sexual neurasthenia is discriminating and judicious." CHEMISTRY, SKIN, AND VENEREAL DISEASES. 15 American Pocket Dictionary rou,Recenuy01;.?edised The American Pocket Medical Dictionary. Edited by W. A. Newman Dorland, M. D., Assistant Obstetrician to the Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania. Containing the pronunciation and definition of the principal words used in medicine and kindred sciences. Flexible leather, with gold edges, $1.00 net ; with thumb index, #1.25 net. James W. Holland, M. D., Professor of Medical Chemistry and Toxicology, and Dean, Jefferson Medical College, Philadelphia, " I am struck at once with admiration at the compact size and attractive exterior. I can recommend it to our students without reserve." Stelwagon's Essentials of Skin 8bH^X\£SSM Essentials of Diseases of the Skin. By Henry W. Stel- wagon, M. D., Ph.D., Professor of Dermatology in the Jeffer- son Medical College, Philadelphia. Post-octavo of 276 pages, with 72 text-illustrations and 8 plates. Cloth, $1.00 net. In Saunders' Question- Comp end Series. The Medical News " In line with our present knowledge of diseases of the skin. . . . Continues to main- tain the high standard of excellence for which these question compends have been noted." Wolffs Medical Chemistry ""S^S^S?*"1 Essentials of Medical Chemistry, Organic and Inorganic. Containing also Questions on Medical Physics, Chemical Physiol- ogy, Analytical Processes, Urinalysis, and Toxicology. By Law- rence Wolff, M. D., Late Demonstrator of Chemistry, Jefferson Medical College. Revised by Smith Ely Jelliffe, M. D., Ph.D., Professor of Pharmacognosy, College of Pharmacy of the City of New York. Post-octavo of 222 pages. Cloth, #1.00 net. In Saunders' Question- Comp end Series. New York Medical Journal " The author's careful and well-studied selection of the necessary requirements of the student has enabled him to furnish a valuable aid to the student." Martin's Minor Surgery, Bandaging, and the Venereal Diseases Second Edition, Revised Essentials of Minor Surgery, Bandaging, and Venereal Diseases. By Edward Martin, A. M., M. D., Professor of Clin- ical Surgery, University of Pennsylvania, etc. Post-octavo, 166 pages, with 78 illustrations. Cloth, #1.00 net. In Saunders' Question- Compend Series. The Medical News "The best condensation of the subjects of which it treats yet placed before the pro' fession." 16 URINE, EYE, EAR, NOSE, AND THROAT. Wolfs Examination of Urine A Laboratory Handbook of Physiologic Chemistry and Urine-examination. By Charles G. L. Wolf, M. D., Instructor in Physiologic Chemistry, Cornell University Medical College, New York. i2mo volume of 204 pages, fully illustrated. Cloth, #1.25 net. British Medical Journal " The methods of examining the urine are very fully described, and there are at the end of the book some extensive tables drawn up to assist in urinary diagnosis." Jackson's Essentials of Eye Third Revised Edition Essentials of Refraction and of Diseases of the Eye. By Edward Jackson, A. M., M. D., Emeritus Professor of Diseases of the Eye, Philadelphia Polyclinic. Post-octavo of 261 pages, 82 illus- trations. Cloth, $1.00 net. In Saunders' Question- Compend Series. Johns Hopkins Hospital Bulletin " The entire ground is covered, and the points that most need careful elucidation are made clear and easy." Gleason's Nose and Throat Third Edition, Revised Essentials of Diseases of the Nose and Throat. By E. B. Gleason, S. B., M. D., Clinical Professor of Otology, Medico- Chirurgical College, Philadelphia, etc. Post-octavo, 241 pages, 1 12 illustrations. Cloth, $1.00 net. In Saunders' Question Compends, The Lancet, London " The careful description which is given of the various procedures would be sufficient to enable most people of average intelligence and of slight anatomical knowledge to make a very good attempt at laryngoscopy." Gleason's Diseases of the Ear Third Edition, Revised Essentials of Diseases of the Ear. By E. B. Gleason, S. B., M. D., Clinical Professor of Otology, Medico-Chirurgical College, Phila., etc. Post-octavo volume of 214 pages, with 114 illustra- tions. Cloth, $ 1. 00 net. In Saunders' Question- Compend Series. Bristol Medico-Chirurgical Journal " We know of no other small work on ear diseases to compare with this, either in freshness of style or completeness of information." Wilcox on Genito-Urinary and Venereal Diseases IssJuued Essentials of Genito-Urinary and Venereal Diseases. By Starling S. Wilcox, M. D., Professor of Genito-Urinary Diseases and Syphilology, Starling Medical College, Columbus, Ohio. i2mo of 313 pages, illustrated. Cloth, $1.00 net. Saunders1 Compends. UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA MEDICAL SCHOOL LIBRARY THIS BOOK IS DUE ON THE LAST DATE STAMPED BELOW UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA MEDICAL SCHOOL LIBRARY