Cathleen Martyniak UF Dissertation Project Preservation Department University of Florida Libraries P.O. Box 117008 Gainesville, FL 32611-7008

Dear Ms. Martyniak:

I am pleased to learn of the UF Dissertation Project, and to make my "aged" dissertation available for researchers. However, there are two errors that must be corrected to successfully raise larvae of the lesser cornstalk borer using the methods listed. On page 13, the first sentence of the first full paragraph should read:

Each cage with pupae was numbered and put in a culture room at 30 + 1 degrees centigrade, 30 % relative humidity, and daily photoperiod of 13 firs light.

I hereby authorize you or whomever you appoint to make these changes.

Thank you for your assistance. I wish you the best of success with this new project.

Sincerely,

Karl J. Stone

REPRODUCTIVE BIOLOGY OF THE

LESSER CORNSTALK BORER,

ELASMOPALPUS L IG NO SELL US (ZELLER)

(LEPIDOPTERA: PHYCITIDAE)

By KARL JOHNSON STONE

A DISSERTATION PRESENTED TO THE GRADUATE COUNCIL OF

THE UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA

IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUffiEMENTS FOR THE

DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY

UNIVERSITY OF FLORIDA 1968

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

I greatly appreciate the advice and criticism offered by Dr. T. J. Walker, chairman of my supervisory committee, during the research and preparation of the dissertation,.

Appreciation is gratefully extended to Dr. L. A. Hetrick and Dr. J. T. Creighton, Deparrment of Entomology; Dr. D. B. Ward, Department of Botany; Dr. G. C. LaBrecque, United States Department of Agriculture, Entomology Re- search Division; and Dr. H. K. Wallace, Chairman of the Department of Zoology, who served as members of the supervisory committee.

Appreciation is extended to Dr. W. G. Eden, Chairman of the Department of Entomology for providing assistants who helped maintain the insect colony.

Special thanks is expressed to Mr. J. Beckner, Department of Botany for his assistance in botanical nomenclature, and to Mr. P. U. Roos for assistance in translating French and German material.

Sincere gratitude is extended to my wife for her generous assistance, patience, and constant encouragement involving long hours end many sacrifices.

TABLE OF CONTENTS

Page

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ii

LIST OF TABLES v

LIST OF FIGURES vi

INTRODUCTION ]

REVIEW OF LITERATURE 3

General References 3

Host Plants r"3)

MATERIALS AND METHODS 12

Rearing Techniques 12

General Methods 12

Aberrant Pupae 19

Other Materials and Methods 21

MORPHOLOGICAL STUDIES 22

Morphology of the Reproductive System 22

Materials and Methods 22

Results and Discussion 22

Male 22

Female 26

The Spermatophore 29

Materials and Methods 31

Results and Discussion 31

Primary Simplex and Spermatophore Color 33

Materials and Methods 33

Results and Discussion 34

Egg Development and Position Relative to Age 35

Materials and Methods 37

Results and Discussion 37

iii

Morphology of the Tympanic Organ 39

Materials and Methods 40

Results and Discussion 40

BEHAVIORAL STUDIES 45

Mating Cage Conditions 45

Materials and Methods 45

Results and Discussion 46

Mating Behavior 48

Materials and Methods 49

Results and Discussion 50

Influence of Additional Females on Male Mating Frequency ... 54

Materials and Methods 54

Results and Discussion 55

Influence of Age on Mating 55

Materials and Methods 56

Results and Discussion 56

Influence of Male Antennectomy on Mating 56

Materials and Methods 58

Results and Discussion 58

Longevity of Virgin and Mated Moths, Spermatophore Passage and

Acceptance, and Fecundity 58

Materials and Methods 59

Results and Discussion 60

Time of Oviposition 76

Materials and Methods 77

Results and Discussion 77

Response of Adults to Sound 77

Materials and Methods 80

Results and Discussion 82

SUMMARY 85

LITERATURE CITED 88

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH 95

LIST OF TABLES

Table Page

1 Reported host plants of the lesser cornstalk borer 6

2 Composition of medium for lesser cornstalk borer larvae 16

3 Color fluid in the 1st secretory area of the primary simplex of 3-

day-old mated and unmated lesser cornstalk borer males at dif- ferent periods of the day following mating the previous night. . . 36

4 Cage conditions and spermatophores passed by 2-dcy-old fed and

unfed lesser cornstalk borer adults, tested for 4 nights in 40-dr

vials, 1 pair per vial, 22 replicates per test 47

5 Longevity of lesser cornstalk borer adults 61

6 Spermatophore passage and acceptance during the lifetime of various

Lepidoptera 63

7 Fecundity of the lesser cornstalk borer. 69

8 Lesser cornstalk borer females showing 2 or more variations from

basic population oviposition patterns 73

LIST OF FIGURES

Figure Page

1 Cages used in rearing technique. A. Matlng-oviposifion cage.

B. Rearing cage 14

2 VeniTal view of terminal pupal abdominal segments of the lesser

cornstalk borer. 18

3 Reproductive system of the male lesser cornsfalk borer 25

4 Reproductive system of the female lesser cornstalk borer. ... 28

5 Spermatophore of the lesser cornstalk borer 32

6 Egg development and position in the reproductive tract of lesser

cornstalk borer virgin females relative to age 38

7 External anterior view of the first abdominal segment of the lesser

cornstalk borer moth illustrating the tympanic organs on the ex- cised abdomen. 43

8 Internal lateral view of the right tympanic organ of the lesser

cornstalk moth with the lateral wall of the tympanic sac removed . 44

9 Percent mating of 1-6-day-old lesser cornstalk borer adults caged

for 1 day 57

10 Mated lesser cornstalk borer male longevity and spermatophores

passed 64

11 Mated lesser cornstalk borer female longevity, oviposition period,

fecundity, and spermatophores accepted 65

12 Total number of spermatophores passed per day by 25 lesser corn-

stalk borer males, number of males after day 14 as indicated. . . 67

13 Average numbers of eggs laid per day by 25 lesser cornstalk borer

mated females, number of females after day 12 cs indicated. . . 71

vi

F?9ure Page

14 Diurnal and nocturnal response of lesser cornstalk borer adults

to various amplitudes and frequencies 84

INTRODUCTION

The lesser cornstalk borer, Elosmopalpus lignosellus (Zeller), is an important pest of crops in Florida, causing considerable damage to corn, soybeans, peanuts, sugar cane, field peas, southern peas, and rye. The moth is widely distributed throughout the tropical and temperate regions of the New World, including the southern half of the United States from California to the Carol inas, north on the East Coast to Massachusetts, and south thru Central America and South America to Argentina, Chile, end Peru (Heinrich, 1956).

Damage to crops in Florida occurs primarily in areas with muck or sandy soils (Strayer, J. R. ' 1968. Personal communication.). Luginbill and Ainslie (1917) and Lyle (1927) found that damage usually seems greater on thin sandy soils in South Carolina, Florida, and Mississippi. King et al. (1961) in Texcs reported damage is especially severe during drought periods. Leuck (1966) in Georgia reported larvae damage seedlings during drought and when late plantings are followed by hot dry periods. Wide-spread infestations have led to several insecticidal investigations.

However, little is known about the moth's reproductive biology. Basic research in this area was facilitated by rearing the insect in the laboratory and examining the reproductive morphology with respect to structure and changes occurring when moths

Assistant Extension Entomologist, Institute of Food and Agricultural Sciences, University of Florida.

2

mated or aged. In addition the morphology of the tympanic organ is discussed.

Behavioral studies included mating behavior and various factors which influ- enced this activity: cage conditions, additional females on male mating frequency, age, and male antennectomy. The longevity of virgin and mated moths, sperma- tophore passage patterns, and fecundity were studied, as well as time of ovipo- sition. Lastly, the response of adults to sound was observed.

The above information may facilitate studies on the effects of chemosterilants, antimetabolites, and predator and parasite relationships.

REVIEW OF LITERATURE

General References

The literature contains considerable research on insecticidal control of the lesser cornstalk borer, but little is known about its biology. Below are the papers that deal with reproductive biology and rearing methods.

Luginbill and Ainslie (1917), using moths from South Carolina and Florida, dis- cussed rearing methods and mating cage conditions, as well as longevity, fecundity, and time of mating. Sanchez (1960), Dupree (1965), Leuck (1966), and Calvo (1966) discussed similar factors with moths from Texas, Georgia, and Florida.

Host Plants

The lesser cornstalk borer attacks many weeds and crops, especially legumes and grasses, throughout the southern half of the United States (King et al., 1961).

Table 1 lists 62 host plants of the lesser cornstalk borer. The following 14 families are represented: Chencpodiaceae, Convolvulaceae, Cruciferae, Cucur- bitaceae, Cyperaceae, Gramineae, Iridaceae, Leguminosae, Linaceae, Malvaceae, Pinaceae, Rosaceae, Rutaceae, and Solanaceae. Confusion in the taxonomy of culti- vated plants and common names complicated compilation of the list, and in some cases, the same host is listed under different common names to indicate common usage in different parts of the country. In such cases, e.g. black-eyed beans = black-eyed

3

4 peas, the host is counted only once in the total count of hosts. Where varietal names are available, each host is counted once. Scientific names were obtained from standard references (Bailey, 1949; Fernald, 1950; Hitchcock, 1951) and Hill's (1937) paper on cultivated sorghums.

Most authors consider the pest a subterranean feeder, but some reports suggest feeding on the aerial parts of plants. Similar feeding behavior of other species may have misled workers and confused host records. For instance, the phycitid moth, Ufa rubedinella (Zeller) (=Elasmopalpus rubedinellus (Zeller)), redescribed by Heinrich (1956) was reported feeding on leaves and fruits of lima beans and black-eyed peas in Florida (unpublished records, Florida Department of Agriculture, Division of Plant Industry, 1944-1945). The larva webs debris in a manner similar to the lesser cornstalk borer, but the insect has not been reported in the DPI files since 1945. Unidentified pyralid larvae were reported on peach seedlings by Dekle (1965) with the characteristic sand-debris subterranean tunnels of E. lignosellus; however, the tunnels also extended up the stems and over leaves.

Table 1 Notes

a U.S. Dep. Agr. , 1952-1968

b Reynolds, Anderson, and Andres, 1959

c Chitfenden, 1900

d Chittenden, 1903b

e Howard, 1900

f Vorhiesand Wehrle, 1946

g Isely and Miner, 1944

h Wilson and Keisheimer, 1955

i Keisheimer, 1955

j Bissell, 1945

k Bissell, 1946

I Lyle, 1927

m Luginbill and Ainslie, 1917

n Sanchez, 1960

o Riley, 1882 (as cited by Luginbill and Ainslie, 1917.)

p Riley, 1882a

q Riley, 1882b

r Dempsey and Brantley, 1953

s Forbes, 1905

t Webster, 1906

u Bissell and Dupree, 1947

v Leuck, 1966

w Chittenden, 1903a

x Heinrich, 1956

y Dupree, 1964

z King, Harding, and Langley, 1961

aa Arthur and Arant, 1956

bb Walton, Matlock, and Boyd, 1964

cc Cunningham, King, and Langley, 1959

dd Harding, 1960

ee Calvo, 1966

ff Cowart and Dempsey, 1949

gg Ash and Bibby, 1957

hh Stahl, 1930

Table l.-Repcried host plants of the lesser cornstalk borer

field

green

Scientific Name

Medicago sativa L.

Hordeum vulgare L. Phaseolus sp.

Phaseolus I i mens is Mac fad.

Phaseolus vulgaris L.

Localities

References

Ariz., Kans, Tex.

Calif.

b.

Ala., Ariz. , Ark. , a,b,c,d,e, Calif., Dela., f,g,h,i,j,

Fla., Ga., Md., k,l. Miss., Mo., N.C., Okla., S.C., Tenn. , Tex. , Va.

N.C.

Vigna sinensis (L.) Endl.

Calif.

Phaseolus vulgaris L. var. humilis Alef.

Tex.

Okla. a.

Ala., Fla., Ga. , a. Md., S.C., Va.

Tex. a.

Phaseolus vulgaris L,

Ariz.

Beta vulgaris L.

Fla.

Ala., Ark., Conn. , Ga. , Md. N.C, Okla., S. C. , Tenn. , Va.

Calif., Ga.

Ala.

Table 1 Continued

Common Name

Scientific Name

Localities

References

Cabbage

Brassica oleracea L. var. capitata L.

Fla.

a.

Cane,

Saccharum officinarium

Fla.

m.

Japanese

L.

sugar

Fla., La. , Miss.

a,l.

Cantaloupe

Cucumis melo L. var. cantalupensis Naud.

Calif., Tex.

a,n.

Chufa

Cyperus esculentus L. var. sativus Boeckl.

Fla.

m.

Citrus

Citrus sp.

Fla.

a.

Clover, crimson

Trifolium incarnatum L. var. elatius Gibelli & Belli

Ga.

J-

Clover, White Dutch

Trifolium repens L.

Fla.

a.

Cole crops

Brassica sp.

Va.

a.

Corn

Zea mays L.

Ala., Ark.,

a,b,f,g,h,

field silage

i / /"■/ "/

Conn., Fla., Ga. , o,p,q,r,s.

III., La., Mass.,

Md., Miss., N.C.,

Okla., S.C., Tex.,

Va.

Fla., Ga. , Tex. a.

Ga. a.

Corn, broom

Sorghum vulgare Pers. var. technicum (Koern.) Fiori & Paoletti

Okla.

Table 1 Continued

Scientific Name

Localities

References

Sorghum vulgare Pers. Calif.

var. caffrorum (Retz.) Hubbard & Render

Zea mays L. var. rugosa Bonaf.

Ariz., Calif., Fla.

a.

Hibiscus gossypium L.

Calif.

b.

Vigna sinensis ~fL.) Endl.

Ala., Ariz. , Ark. , Fla. , Ga. , Miss. , N.C., Okla., S.C., Tex., Va.

a/d,f,g,h, i,j,k,l,m, t,u,v,w,x.

Linum usitatissimum L.

X.

Alopecurus pratensis L.

Ark.

g.

Gladiolus sp.

Fla.

i.

Echinochloa crusgalli (L.) Beauv.

Ark.

g.

Cynodon dactylon (L.) Pers.

Calif., Ga.

b,y.

Panicum texanum Buckl.

Tex.

a.

Diqitaria sanquinalis (L.) Scop.

Ark., Calif., Fla. , Ga. , Tex. , Md.

a,b,g,h,n,

Sorghum halepense (L.) Pers.

Ariz. , Calif. , Fla. , Miss. , Okla., Tex.

a,b,h,n.

Cyperus esculentus L.

Calif., Fla.

b,i.

Digitaria decumbens

Fla.

a.

Stent.

Table 1 Continued

Common Name

Scientific Nar

Localities

References

Grass, Rhodes

Grass, rye

Grass, Sudan

Grass, water

Chloris gayana Kunth Fla.

Lolium sp. Fla.

Sorghum sudanense Tex. (Piper) Stapf

Cyperus sp. Calif.

Grass, wire

Eleusine indica (L.) Gaertn.

Ga., S.C.

Ground burnut

Aegilops sp. ?

Tex.

Hegari

Locust, black

Lupine

Lupine, blue

Cyamopsis psoralioides Tex. D.C.

Sorghum vulgare Pers. Ariz., Tex.

Robinia pseudoacacia L.

Lupinus augustifolius L. Fla.,Ga. var. 'rancher'

Lupinus hirsutus L.

a,n.

Millet

Milo maize

Oars

Oars, wild

Panicum miliaceum L. La., S.C. a.

Sorghum subglabrescens Ariz., Tex. a,m. (Sieud.) A. F. Hill-

Avena sativa L. Ala., Calif., a,b,n.

Miss., S.C. , Tex.

Avena barbata Brot. Calif. b.

or A. fatua L.

Table 1 Continued

10

Common Name

Scientific Name

Localities

References

Papyrus

Cyperus papyrus L.

Calif.

a.

Peach

Prunus persica (L.) Batsch

Fla.

a.

Peanuts

Arachis hypogaea L.

Ala. , Ariz. , Calif., Fla., Ga„ , Miss. , N.C., Okla., S.C, Tex.

a,c,e,f,h, i,l,n,y,z,

aa,bb,cc, dd.

Peas

Pisum sativum L.

Ala., Ariz. , Ark., Calif., Fla., Ga., N.C., Okla., S.C, Tex.

a,b.

garden

Ala.

a.

winter

Tex.

a.

Peas, Austrian

Pisum sativum L. var. arvense (L.) Poir.

Tex.

a.

winter

field

Ala., Calif. , Fla., Ga. , Miss. , Tex.

a,n.

Peas, black-eyed

Vigna sinensis (L.) Endl.

Ala., Ariz. , Calif. , Ga. , Tex.

a,n.

southern

Ala.

a.

southern table

Ala.

a.

Pimento

Capsicum frutescens L.

Dela. , Fla., Ga.

a,j,i,r,ff.

Pine, loblolly

Pinus toeda L.

Va.

a.

Potato,

sweet

Ipomoea baralas Lam.

Calif., Fla., Ga.

a.

Table 1 Continued

11

Common

Name

Scientific Name

Localities

References

Rice

Oryza sativa L.

Fla., La.

a.

Rye

Secale cereale L.

Fla., Tex.

a.

Sorghum

Sorghum vulgare Pers. var. vulgare

Ariz. , Calif. , Fla., Ga. , La. ,

a,b,f,l,m, t.

Miss., Okla., S.C, Tex.

Sorghum, grain

Sorghum vulgare Pers. Ala., Ariz., var. vulgare Fla., Miss.,

N.C., Okla., S.C.

a/gg.

Sorghum, Sudan

Sorghum almum Parodi Ga.

Soybeans

Stock, garden

Glycine max (L.) Merr. Ala., Ariz., a, v.

Ark., Fla„, Ga. , La., Miss., N.C., S.C, Tex., Va.

Mafthiola sp.

Calif.

b.

Strawberries

Fragaria virginiana Ala., Ark., a,g,i,hh.

"Duch. Calif., Fla., Md. ,

N.C., N.Y.,

Tenn. , Va.

Tomatoes

Lycopersicon esculentum Fla., N.C. , Tex. a. ~MiTL

Turnips

Brass ice napus L.

Ala. , Ariz. Fla., Ga.

i,f,h,m.

Vetch Wheat

Vicia sp.

Tex.

Triticum oestivum L. Ala., Fla., a,h,m.

Okla. , Tenn. , Tex.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Rearing Techniques General Methods

Moths collected by Calvo (1966) in light traps near Gainesville, Florida, were the original stock for this investigation. Individuals from this stock were used to start a colony which was maintained for 7 months to establish a large colony before experimentation began. The colony was maintained for 27 months thereafter, representing a total of approximately 32 generations.

Several workers reared the insect thru its life history for 1 or 2 generations (Luginbill and Ainslie, 1917; Sanchez, 1960; KingetaL, 1961; Dupree, 1965; Leuck, 1966). Calvo (1966) reared several generations on a modification of Berger's (1963) diet for Heliothis sp. Larvae were reared individually in vials. Macerated corn seedlings were added to the diet every 3rd generation to avoid pupal aberrations. Calvo (1966) collected eggs laid on paper thru screen topped cages the method adopted here.

Transparent plastic containers, 10x10x7 1/2 cm deep were used as mating- oviposition cages (Fig. 1 A). Ten male and ten female pupae were placed in each cage. A 14 x 18 mesh per inch galvanized screen stapled to a rim of wooden strips was placed over the cage mouth. Two percent sucrose solution was supplied in a wide-mouth pipette and soft rubber bulb (total capacity 4 ml) placed thru a hole

12

13 in the screen. This supply normally lasted 12 days. Immediately below the pipette was a dish 4 cm in diameter/ held in place by masking tape in a rear floor corner. The dish trapped sugar solution that might drop from the pipette. If spilled solution coated the pupae, the adults could not emerge properly. Four mating-oviposition cages were prepared on each of 5 days during the week.

Each cage with pupae was numbered and put in a culture room at 47 + 1 C, 30-50% relative humidity, and daily photoperiod of 13 hr light. The pipette bulb was squeezed daily to release the air bubble that formed at the bottom, thus mak- ing the solution available to the emerged moths. After 1 of each sex had emerged, a paper sheet identified with the corresponding cage number was placed daily over the screen top. One corner of the egg sheet was folded upward to allow space for the protruding pipette bulb. The plastic cage top was placed diagonally over the sheet to hold it down. Eggs were laid on the sheet through the screen and were readily visible on the paper. After 7 of each sex emerged, this procedure was con- tinued for 9 more days.

Egg sheets were removed daily and set aside on frays in the culture room for 24 hr. Egg color was used to differentiate between fertile and sterile eggs, since fertile eggs turned from cream white to red, while sterile eggs remained cream color- ed or turn red at 1 end only. The sheets were arranged in order on the basis of mating cage number and those with less than 30 eggs or more than 10% sterile eggs were discarded. To preserve genetic variability, the following system was used. Sixteen sheets were selected from the remaining egg sheets and divided into 4 groups of 4 each, with the lowest numbered sheet going to the 1st group, the next highest to the 2nd group, and so on until distributed. Then 25 fertile eggs were cut from

14

Fig. 1. -Cages used in rearing technique. A. Maring-oviposition cage. B. Rearing cage.

15

each sheer and each group was pur into a transparent plastic rearing cage. This cage (Fig. 1 B), 12 1/4 x 17 1/4 cm by 6 cm deep was filled to about 12 mm depth with medium, modified from Berger (1963).

A duo-speed Waring^blender, model 1002, with 1 liter containers was used in media preparation. A blend was mixed in the order listed in Table 2. Blending was at low speed thru and including addition of alphacel. Previously measured ingredients were added without pause to avoid a highly viscous mixture. While the agar cooled to about 41 C after removal from the autoclave (about 3 min, running cool tap water over the flask containing the agar), the blender was run continuously. The agar was then added with the blender running at high speed for the remaining blending. The total mixing process took 6-8 min.

Medium prepared the day before use and placed under refrigeration was best, as fresh medium had somewhat more moisture than optimum. Any moisture that con- densed on the sides of the container when it was removed from the refrigerator was wiped off. Otherwise, the water would cover the medium and ultimately kill the eggs, young larvae, or both. About 250 ml of sifted, white, sterilized sand was poured down the long axis of each cage, and the sand ridge was leveled off, leav- ing the medium free of sand along the cage sides. The papers with 100 eggs of each group were placed on the sand ridges. Next a sterilized paper towel was placed over the cage rim, and a screen top was placed firmly over the towel. The screen top was made by cutting out the center of the plastic container top and replacing it with screen of the same mesh as used for the mating cages. The towel reduced evaporation and the screen top prevented larval escape. To reduce evaporation further, a 1.3 cm thickness of sterilized cellucotton was put over the screen top and heid on with a large rubber band.

16 Table 2. -Composition of medium for lesser cornstalk borer larvae.

Distilled water 190.0 ml

KOH, 22.5% 4.3 ml

Casein, vitamin free 40.0 g

Wesson's salts 8.5 g

Sucrose 22.7 g

Formaldehyde, 10% 3. 1 ml Solution of: 7 g methyl p-hydroxybenzoate and

7 g sorbic acid in 50 ml 95% ethyl alcohol 12.5 ml

Wheat germ 25.6 g

Alphacel (hydrolyzed purified cellulose, powder) 4.3 g

Agar, dissolved in 515 ml water 21.3 g

Vitamin diet fortification mixture0 8.5 g

Ascorbic acid 13.6 g

Streptomycin sulfate (700 micrograms/ml) 118.0 mg

a Nutritional Biochemicals Corporation, Cleveland, Ohio

17

After 21 days, the cage was opened, and the cellucotton, paper towel, and medium were removed. The screen top was replaced and used as a sieve for rapid separation of cocoons from the sand. The cocoons were hand picked from the re- maining debris and placed in a small household sieve.

The sieve with the cocoons were dipped in dilute Chlorox^ solution (1 part chlorox to 1 part water) for 45-60 sec and agitated to dissolve the silk and free the pupae (Bartlett and Martin, 1945). After rinsing the pupae in water, the pupae were dipped for 15-30 sec in 60% isopropyl alcohol. The pupae sank and the lar- val exuviae were floated off. The pupae were spread on a paper towel to dry.

Separation plus extraction took about 15 min per cage.

After extraction and drying, the pupae were sexed by examining the terminal abdominal segment (Fig. 2). Sexing was completed in 15 min per cage. Pupae were then selected for mating-oviposition cages. No more than 5 pupae and no more than 3 of 1 sex from a given rearing cage were put in 1 mating-oviposition cage. Pupae at the same developmental stage, judged by color, were placed in a given mating-oviposition cage so that adults would emerge about the same time. Four cages per day 5 days a week were prepared, numbered, and put in the culture room .

Females began ovipositing on the 2nd night after emergence. During the 9 days that a mating-oviposition cage was kept, about 80% of the eggs that would be laid v/ere deposited on the egg sheers. Colony daily egg production was about 5000. Duration of egg, larval, or pupal stages was not recorded as production of adults was the primary goal. Total time from egg deposition to adult emergence was 24-28 days. Larvae consumed about 1/10 of the media. Production was low

Fig. 2. -Ventral view of terminal pupal abdominal segments of the lesser cornstalk borer

19

for the 1st few generations, but after a few generations, 60 to 80 pupae were ob- tained per cage.

The average duration from egg to adult, 26 days, was as short or shorter than results reported to date. Luginbill and Ainslie (1917) in South Carolina recorded 38.5 days for the spring generation, and 64.6 days for the fall generation under unspecified laboratory conditions, feeding larvae cowpea leaves. Spring temper- atures ranged somewhere between 80-90 F diurnally and reached 80 F noctur- nally. Sanchez (1960) in Texas reported 24.3 days during August, and 46.3 days during September-October, feeding larvae peanut roots. Eggs were exposed to 70-100 F. Larvae were exposed to the following average daily minimum and maximum temperatures: June, 77 and 92.3 F; July, 79.5 and 9o F; August, 79.5 and 94.5 F; September, 74 and 89 F. Pupae were exposed to temperature ranges between the following minimum and maximum temperatures: July, 76-100 F; August, 66-102 F; September, 64 and 98 F. No temperature ranges were mentioned for October. King et al. (1961) relied heavily on Sanchez for data and gave little detail on methods. Dupree (1965) in Georgia recorded 47.8 days and 55 days during June-September in 1957 and 1958, respectively, feeding larvae foliage and stem sections of seedling southern peas. Average minimum and maxi- mum temperatures were 66.6 and 86.4° F in 1957, and 66.8 and 88.2° F in 1958. Leuck (1966) in Georgia reported 32.8 4- 2.3 days, feeding larvae soybeans or cowpea leaves. Monthly mean minimum temperatures ranged from 57.4 and 72.7° F, monthly mean maximum 79.7 and 96.2 F.

Aberrant Pupae

Calvo (1966) found after rearing 3 successive generations, a small but unspecified

20 percentage of pupae appeared with poorly developed wing pads and light scleroti- zation over the pupal 3rd and 4th abdominal segments posterior to the wing pads. He found that the addition of macerated corn seedlings to the diet every 3rd gen- eration eliminated pupal aberrations.

To determine the percent pupal aberrations and percent successful emergence from aberrant pupae occurring with my rearing technique after 23 generations, the following experiment was conducted.

Groups of pupae extracted on each of 6 days were examined. Aberrant pupae were described as below and place in numbered 4-dr vials, and the vials with pupae were placed in the culture room until adult emergence or death. Dead pupae be- came discolored and shriveled. Normal pupae not used in colony maintanence were used as controls and were handled in the same manner but not numbered.

All aberrations were on the pupal venter. These 4 categories v/ere recognized: (A) a lightly sclerofized area between the 3rd ana/or 4th abdominal segment and the wing pads and appendages; (B) a lightly sclerotized area between the 3rd and/or 4th abdominal segment and 1 or more appendages but not wing pads; (C) a lightly sclerotized area between 2 or more appendages or between a wing pad and append- age; and (D) 1 or more bright green appendages in an otherwise uniformly colored pupa.

Of 1721 pupae extracted, 5% of the male and 6% of the female pupae had aberrations. Fifteen percent of aberrant pupae exhibited 2 or more aberration types. Seventy-two percent of the males and 84% of the females emerged successfully, i.e., with wings fully expended and with normally formed appendages. In the controls, 93% of the males and 92% of the females emerged successfully.

21

The low percent pupal aberrations and the high percent successful emergence from aberrant pupae indicated the rearing technique was adequate.

Other Materials and Methods

Specific materials and methods are outlined under the appropriate sections be- low. However, a few general procedures that were used in several experiments are mentioned here.

Pupae not used to maintain the colony were placed in 4-dr 20 x 70 mm glass shell vials, each with a small wad of cotton, and the vials were stoppered with cotton plugs. When moths emerged, the cotton wad was saturated with 2% sucrose solution. Adults were used the same day they emerged or were held in the 4-dr vials until they reached an age desired for experimentation.

In experiments involving mating and longevity of virgins, moths were placed in 4.7 cm x 8.5 cm 40-dr clear plastic vials. A wad of cellucotton was placed in the vial bottom and was saturated with 2% sucrose solution. The vial mouths were closed with squares of cellucotton 1/2 mm thick held in place with rubber bands. The vials with moths were then placed vertically in enameled pans and the pans with vials were placed in the temperature-controlled culture room. In experi- ments in which the moths were held for more than 4 days, clean vials and saturated cellucotton wads were provided daily.

All experiments were conducted in the same controlled temperature room at 47 4 C, 30-50% RH, and a daily photoperiod of 13 hr light beginning at 7:00 AM.

With all morphological drawings, a stereoscopic microscope with an eyepiece grid was used in making measurements.

MORPHOLOGICAL STUDIES

Morphology of the Reproductive System In order to fully understand certain aspects of mating behavior, the infernal reproductive morphology of the lesser cornstalk borer needed examination. The literature contains no reference to the subject. However, other workers have examined several related species, which are compared with E. lignosellus below.

Materials and Methods

Five 1 -day-old adults of each sex were dissected in physiological saline con- sisting of NaCI 8 gm, KCI 0.2 gm, CaCI2 0.2 gm, H20 to liter.

Results and Discussion

Male

The male reproductive systems of E. lignosellus and the phycifid species Anagasta kuehniella (Zeller) (Mediterranean flcjr moth), Ephestia cautella (Walker) (almond moth), Ephestia ellutella (Hubner) (tobacco moth), and Plodia interpunctella (Hubner) (lnd;an-meal moth) (Norris, 1932) are very similar, but the ductus ejacu- latorius simplex and cornutus of E. lignosellus differ from the others.

The unpaired ductus ejaculatorius simplex extends from the caudal end of the ductus ejaculatorius duplex (Fig. 3, D.e.d) to the aedeagus (Fig. 3, A). In the Noctuidae, Callahan (1958b) and Callahan and Chapin (1960) divided the duct

22

23 into a cephalad primary secretory region and a caudad cuticular region where sperma- tophore precursors are molded. Later Callahan and Cascio (1963) divided the primary secretory region into the 1st and 2nd secretory areas. Norris (1932) histologically determined 4 secretory regions in the ducts of A. kuehniella, Ephestia spp. , and P. interpunctella, while Musgrave (1937) recognized 8 regions in A. kuehniella.

In E. lignosellus, 4 regions are present in the duct, based on exterior gross morphology (Fig. 3). They are described below using the terminology of Norris (1932) with the terminology of Callahan and Cascio (1963) in parentheses.

The 1st region, which appears to include the 1st 3 unpaired glands, (=2nd secretory area of the primary simplex) extends approximately 11.5 mm (diameter, 0.15 mm) from the ductus ejaculatorius duplex (Fig. 3, P.s.2).

The 2nd region or 4th unpaired gland (-1st secretory area of the primary sim- plex) extends 2.0 mm (diameter 0.2 mm) (Fig. 3, P.s. 1). The region is not as great- ly inflated with respect to the 1st 3 unpaired glands in A. kuehniella, Ephestia spp., and P. interpunctella.

The 3rd region or ductus ejaculatorius is 4 mm long (Fig. 3, D.e). The in- flated 4th region is the bulbus ejaculatorius (=cuticular simplex) (Fig. 3, B.e). The elongated horns of the ductus ejaculatorius of A. kuehniella, Ephestia spp., and IP. interpunctella are replaced in E. lignosellus by 2 swollen structures in the same position as the horns (Fig. 3, l.c), adjacent to the aedeagus (Fig. 3, A).

The cornutus of E. lignosellus is a slender curved tooth (Fig. 3, C) differing from the thickened teeth of various shapes of A. kuehniella, Ephestia spp., and P. interpunctella.

The morphology of the testes of E. lignosellus is the same as in A. kuehniella,

Explanation of Fig. 3

Terminology mostly after Norris (1932) Terminology after Callahan and Cascio (1963) in parentheses

A Aedeagus

A.g Acessory glands

B.e Bulbus ejaculatorius (of cuticular simplex) of ductus ejaculatorius simplex

C Cornutus

D.e Ductus ejaculatorius of ductus ejaculatorius simplex

D.e.d Ductus ejaculatorius duplex

E Endophallus

I.C Inflated chambers of ductus ejaculatorius (of cuticular simplex) of ductus ejaculatorius simplex

P.s. 1 (1st secretory area of the primary simplex) of ductus ejaculatorius simplex

P.s.2 (2nd secretory area of the primary simplex) of ductus ejaculatorius simplex

S.v Seminal vesicle

T Testes

V.d Vas deferens

25

Fig. 3. -Reproductive system of the male lesser cornstalk borer

26 Ephesfia spp., and P. interpunctella. Cholodkovsky (1884) recognized 4 groups of Lepidoptera based on testes types: (A) testes completely separate and 4-lobed, as in Hepialus; (B) testes separate but rounded and 3-lobed, as in Saturnia; (C) dis- cernibly separate testes enclosed in a single scrotum, as in Lycaena; and (D) testes fused and appearing as a single round organ in a common scrotum, as in Pieris. All phycitid species previously mentioned belong to group D.

Female

The female reproductive system of E. lignosellus differs in several details from the phycitid species by Norris (1932).

The bursa copulatrix of the lesser cornstalk borer consists of a sac, the corpus bursae (Fig. 4, C.b), and a neck, the ductus bursae (Fig. 4, D.b), and opens ex- ternally andventrally thru the ostium bursae (Fig. 4, O.b) on the 8th sernite. The corpus bursae bears a large dorsal and small ventral plate bearing approximately 25 and 53 teeth or signa respectively (Fig. 4, D.p.s and V.p.s), which project into the corpus lumen and oppose each other. In P. interpunctella, the 3-8 signa are arranged on the dorsal wall of the corpus, while A. kuehniella has 2-4 signa on the ventral wall only.

The ductus seminalis in E. lignosellus (Fig. 4, D.s) is of uniform diameter for its entire length and apparently lacks a bulla seminis. The duct enters the corpus bursae at a projection on the caudal end of the corpus. In A. kuehniella, Ephestia spp., and P. interpunctella the duct winds around the corpus bursae in close asso- ciation with the corpus wall and opens into the corpus adjacent to the signa, about 1/3 the corpus length from the cephalad end.

The glandula recepfaculi of E. lignosellus (Fig. 4, G.r) is an elongate simple

Explanation of Fig. 4 Terminology after Norris (1932)

A.g Accessory gland "

C Calyx

C.a.g Common duct of accessory gland

C. b Corpus bursae

Co Common oviduct

D.b Ductus bursae with longitudinally ribbed scleroiization

D.p.s Dorsal plate with signa

D.r Ductus receptaculi

D.s Ductus seminal is

G.r Glandula receptaculi

l.v Inflated part of vestibulum wall

L.a.g Lateral duct of accessory gland

L.o Lateral oviduct

M Membrane surrounding the bursae copulatrix and the ductus seminalis

O. b Ostium bursae

Ovar Ovariole

Ovip Ovipositor

R.a.g Reservoir of accessory gland

T.v Terminal vesicle

Va Vagina

Ve Vestibulum

V.p.s Ventral plate with signa

28

V.p.

Fig. 4. -Reproductive system of the female lesser cornstalk borer

29

structure tapering caudally and leading to the coiled ductus receptaculi (Fig. 4, D.r) which in turn opens into a hemispherical inflated portion of the dorsal ves- tibulum wall (Fig. 4, l.v). In P. interpunctella/ the gland is nearly always simple. In A. kuehniella it is often bifurcated at the tip, and one branch may be shorter than the other. In both species, the gland opens into a caudal sac, the recepta- culum seminis, which leads to the ductus receptaculum.

The lesser cornstalk borer accessory glands (Fig. 4, A.g) are elongate struc- tures leading to a series of convolutions that ultimately open into the oval-shaped reservoirs (Fig. 4, R.a.g) which in turn open into narrow ducts leading to the common accessory gland duct (Fig. 4, C.a.g). ln_A. kuehniella, Ephestia spp., and P. interpunctella, the reservoirs are elongate structures with the greater part of the caudal lengths dilated.

The Spermatophore

During a successful mating, the Lepidoptera transfer sperm in a spermatophore formed from secretions in the male ductus ejaculatorius simplex (Callahan, 1958b). Even within families, the spermatophore shape varies greatly (Williams, 1941), and formation and transfer mechanisms are sometimes highly complex (Callahan, 1958b).

A spermatophore of A. kuehniella, E. cautella, E. elutella, or P. interpunctella consists of a rounded corpus with a narrow twisted collum ending in a frenum. Solid horns on the frenum correspond exactly in number and arrangement with species specific structures of the ductus ejaculatorius. The sperm escape thru an oval aper- ture in the frenum with the aperture at the ductus seminal is entrance (Norris, 1932).

Petersen (1907) found that Lepidoptera with ductus seminalis apertures distal on the ductus bursae produce spermatophores with long collums. Where the aperture

30 appears in the corpus bursae, collums are either short or twisted beside the sperma- tophore corpus, as in Anagasta, Ephestia, and Plodia (Norris, 1932).

Williams (1941) divided the Heterocera into 3 groups based on spermatophore communication in the female reproductive system: (A) spermatophores in direct communication with the ductus seminalis, which are found in the majority of the moths; (B) spermatophores communicating with a duct leading to a secretion filled reservoir opening into the ductus seminalis which leads from the reservoir to the vagina, which are found in some arctiids and tortricids; and (C) spermatophores opening into the ductus bursae which extends to the vagina, which are found only in the primitive prodoxids.

Stitz (1901) believed the signa punctured the spermatophore, but Williams (1938) believed spermatophores were dissolved by enzymes in the ductus bursae. Callahan (1958b) stated that there is no evidence for either of these beliefs. He felt Petersen's (1907) theory that the signa serve to hold the smooth plastic-like spermatophore in place is probably correct.

The empty spermatophores in female potato tuberworm moths, Phthorimaea operculella (Zeller), are forced anteriorly in the corpus bursae when multiple mating occurs (Hughes, 1967). Several are found collapsed and nested within each other, while the most recently deposited spermatophore occupied ihe corpus bursae posteriorly.

The purpose of this section is to present the spermatophore morphology of E. lignosellus, its position in the corpus bursae, the probable mode of sperm escape from the spermatophore, and the fate of empty spermatophores.

31

Materials and Methods

Five spermatophores representing 1st matings by both males and females were dissected from females and placed in physiological saline. During dissections, the spermatophore orientation in the corpus bursae was observed.

The spermatophores in multiple mated females were observed in moths taken from 4 mating-oviposition cages. Thirty-two females had 2-5 spermatophores in the corpus bursae.

Results and Discussion

The spermatophore is illustrated in Fig. 5. The frenum bears 2 small rounded projections that seem to correspond to the inflated chambers on the male ductus ejaculatorius (Fig. 3, l.c). The aperture thru which sperm escape is terminal and between the 2 rounded projections.

The 2 toothed plates of the corpus bursae walls tightly hold the spermatophore in place. The collum is twisted and pressed against the posterior lateral wall of the corpus bursae, and the frenum with its aperture is in direct contact with the ductus seminalis. Thus the lesser cornstalk borer belongs to Williams' (1941) group A.

As in the potato tuberworm moth (Hughes, 1967), when multiple mating occurs, empty spermatophores are forced anteriorly in the corpus bursae flattening ana/or nesting into one another. The most recently deposited spermatophore is held tightly between the 2 plates of signa, which seems to support Petersen's (1907) theory of signa function. No spermatophores were punctured and none showed evidence of being dissolved. Callahan (1958b) found striated muscle tissue surrounding the corpus bursae wall of Heliothis zea (Boddie) and theorized constriction of the muscle

32

Corpus

Collum Terminal aperture

Rounded projection Frenum

Fig. 5.-Spermatophore of the lesser cornstalk borer

33

exerted pressure on the spermatophore corpus, thereby forcing sperm out of the struc- ture. This may be the mode of sperm escape from lesser cornstalk borer spermato- phores.

Primary Simplex and Spermatophore Color Snow and Carlysle (1967) reported that the 1st secretory area of the primary simplex in a virgin male fall army worm, Spodoptera frugiperda (J. E. Smith), Is filled with a light brown to black fluid. During mating, portions of the pigment are passed to the female corpus bursae, while the remainder is incorporated into the spermatophore, resulting in a darkly pigmented spermatophore. The simplex is left transparent and colorless-to-yellow. A darkly pigmented spermatophore and a trans- parent, colorless-to-yeilow simplex together indicate a 1st mating for the male. Subsequent spermatophores are clear to yellow.

The technique is limited by age. Newly emerged males have light to medium brown pigment in the simplex. Males mating once and retained 4 days after removal of females have transparent, yellow, or light brown pigment. Virgins of this age have dark brown to black pigment in the simplex and hence are distinguishable from mated males.

The purpose of this experiment was to determine if a color change of the primary simplex fluid indicates a male has mated within 24 hr, and if so, how long the re- sulting color is retained. Spermatophore color was checked to see if those passed 1st differed from those passed subsequently.

Materials and Methods

Each of 182 3-day-old males were caged with two 3-day-old females for 1

34 day. The females were dissected for sperrnatophores. Males were dissecfed for de- termination of simplex color in the morning, afternoon, or evening.

The above experiment indicated virgin males tend to have translucent yellow simplex fluid, while mated males had transparent, colorless-to-yellow fluid. To test this further the following experiments were conducted.

Twenty-five each of 1-, 2-, and 3-day-old virgin males and 10 each of 4-, 5-, and 6-day-old virgin males were dissected for determination of simplex fluid color and light transmission.

Thirty 1-day-old virgin males were caged individually with two 1-3-day-old virgin females for 1 day. Females were dissected for sperrnatophores. Ten mated males were retained 2, 4, and 5 days after removal of females. Five males of each group were dissected for determination of simplex fluid color and light transmission between 9-11 AM and 5 were dissected between 9:30-11 PM.

Ten 1-day-old virgin males were individually caged with two 1-3-day-old virgin females. Females were replaced daily by two 1- to 3-day-old virgin fe- males for 3 days. Females were dissected for sperrnatophores and the color of 1st and subsequent sperrnatophores was compared. Five males were dissected for de- termination of simplex fluid color and light transmission at 1 1 AM and 5 at 9:30 PM.

Materials and methods for further data on simplex color and light transmission and spermatophore color are presented in the section below dealing with mating behavior.

Results and Discussion

Table 3 indicates a progressive color change in simplex fluid color of 3-day-old

35 males within 24 hr after mating. All spermatophores were clear and transparent.

Simplex fluid color was translucent cream-yellow to yellow in 1- to 3-day- old virgin males and trcnslucent yellow in virgin 4- to 6-day-old males.

All mated males retained for 2, 4, and 5 days after removal of females had transparent yellow simplex fluid. Six males not mating the day of caging had translucent yellow fluid the following day.

Nine males mated on each of 3 days when caged with females 3 days. Four males dissected in the morning had transparent colorless simplex fluid. One male failed to mate the 3rd night and had transparent pale yellow simplex fluid. All males dissected in the evening had transparent pale yellow simplex fluid. All spermatophores passed on the 3 nights were clear and transparent.

The above indicates that mated males can be distinguished from virgin males by transparent versus translucent simplex fluid for at least 5 days after mating. A mating during the previous night is indicated by transparent colorless simplex fluid. Color of 1st, 2nd, and 3rd spermatophores passed successively on 3 nights is iden- tical and does not distinguish between 1st and subsequent matings.

Data from the section below dealing with mating behavior indicated the pri- mary simplex fluid of 1-day-old males was colorless and transparent in all mated males and translucent yellow in 2 unmated males. Apparently 1-day-old meted males can be distinguished from unrnated males of the same age within 3 hr of mating.

Egg Development and Position Relative to Age The Lepidoptera show considerable variation in egg development at emergence. Eidmann (1931) placed the Lepidoptera in 3 groups based on the number of full- sized eggs in the ovaries at adult emergence: (A) species with very few full-sized

36

Table 3. -Color of fluid in the 1st secretory area of the primary simplex of

3-day-old mated and unmated lesser cornstalk borer males at different

periods of the day following mating the previous night.

Mated Males Unmated Males

Color of simplex (%) low

Time of

Sample

Col

or of :

iimplex (%)

dissection

Size

Clear

Pale

-yellow

7-12 AM

51

84

2- 3 PM

21

57

5

7- 9 PM

46

33

35

9-11 PM

64

50

Yellow Pale-yellow Yell

16

38

8 2 22

23 27

37

eggs at emergence, typical of butterflies and moths with a long adult life; (B) species having a 2-or 3-fold increase in full-sized eggs in the imago, as with most Heterocera; and (C) species with all eggs fully developed at emergence, as in the Bombycidae and Lymantriidae,

Full-sized eggs are present in ovaries of newly emerged A. kuehniella fe- males, but none are present in P. interpunctella females at emergence (Norris, 1932).

The purpose of this experiment was to determine the stage of egg development and egg position in the reproductive tract of virgin females relative to age.

Materials and Methods

Moths 0-8 hr old and 1 and 2 days old were retained individually in 4-dr vials until dissected in distilled water. Moths were classified into 4 groups; (A) moths with full-sized eggs in the common oviduct, lateral oviducts, calyx, and ovarioles; (B) moths with full-sized eggs in the lateral oviducts, calyx, and ovarioles only; (C) moths with full-sized eggs in the calyx and ovarioles only; and (D) moths with no full-sized eggs. The number of visible eggs in the ovarioles of 0-8-hr-old females were recorded.

Results and Discussion

No full-sized eggs were present in 0-8-hr-old females (Fig. 6) which would place the lesser cornstalk borer in Eidmann's (1931) group A. However, the moth's life span is relatively short (8-22 days) under outside conditions (Dupree, 1965; Leuck, 1966). An average of 26 eggs were visible per ovariole.

Unlike H_. zea (Callahan, 1958b), some E. lignosellus virgin 1-day-old females

38

N =

100

23

90

_

80

-

70

-

60

50

40

-

30

-

20

10

-

0

0-8 hr

N= 56

1 day Age

2 dQN

| Group A. Full-sized eggs In common oviduct, lateral oviducts, calyx, and ovarioles

\Ua Group B. Full-sized eggs in lateral oviducts, calyx, and ovarioles only

b^j Group C. Full-sized eggs in calyx and ovarioles only [ I Group D. No full-sized eggs

Fig. 6. -Egg development and position in reproductive tract of lesser cornstalk borer virgin females relative to age

39

developed full-sized eggs which appeared in the common oviduct, lateral oviducts, ana/or calyx (Fig. 6).

Morphology of the Tympanic Organ The lesser cornstalk borer has tympanic organs. Thoracic and abdominal tympanic organs are found in 11 families of Lepidoptera within the Noctuoidea, the Geometroidea, and the Pyraloidea. As far as known, they are lacking in all other groups (Bourgogne, 1951). The thoracic type is confined to the Noctuoidea, and is considered monophyletic by Kiriakoff (1963). The abdominal organs are divided into 3 groups and appear on the 1st or 2nd segments in the Pyraloidea, the Geometroidea, and the Drepanoidea. The abdominal types ere poorly known but appear polyphyletic in origin. Eggers (1919, 1925, 1928) and Kennel and Eggers (1933) published extensive works on the abdominal organs, but in recent years, these organs have been ignored (Kiriakoff, 1963).

In the Pyraloidea, tympanic organs are found in the 1st abdominal segment, and are often obscure externally since the tympani face the thorax (Bourgogne, 1951). Tympanal cavities are shallow or essentially absent. Principal tympani are situated ventral!-/ on the modified anterior portion of the 1st abdominal ster- nites (sternites 1 and 2), and are separated by a strongly scaled and sclerified longitudinal band. The band frequently continues posteriorly in 2 large projecting lobes that sometimes serve as tympanal opercula. Each tympanic organ has a tympanal sac enclosed in a chitinous hemisphere formed by integumental invagi- nation. Its walls are formed by 2 lamellae which remain completely separated, or may be partly or completely closed. The scolophore (=scolopophorous organ), con- taining 4 scolopalia, may extend from the tympanum to the hemisphere surface

40 or to an internal ridge. In some subfamilies, as many as 5 accessory tympani may be present: (A) a single dorsal tympanum composed of a thin metapostnotal ridge; (B) a pair of lateral tympani on the metathoracic epimera or on either side of the metapostnofum; and (C) a pair of coxal tympani on the posterior face of the meta- thoracic coxae.

Materials and Methods

Five 1-day-old adults of each sex were dissected in distilled water. The thorax was separated from the abdomen, and the 1st abdominal segment bearing the tympanic organs was separated from the remaining abdominal segments. This permitted an unobstructed view of the anterior surfaces. To expose internal struc- ture, an incision was made just posterior and lateral to the anterior margin of the right organ.

Results and Discussion

Figs. 7 and 8 illustrate the external and internal structure of the organs.

There is no sexual dimorphism in the organ structure. The tympanal cavity is essentially absent since only narrow ridges (Fig. 7, S.r) circumscribe the anterior surface of the 1st abdominal segment. The tympanal sacs (Fig. 8) and tympani (Fig. 7, T) are separated by a strongly scaled and sclerified longitudinal band (Fig. 7, S.l.b) which terminates ventral to the oriface thru which the internal organs pass (Fig. 7, O). The metathoracic coxae and the scales on the longi- tudinal band obscure the tympanal surface externally. The principle tympani are ventrally located, and the presumed scolopophorous organs, easily seen thru the thin tympanal integument, pass ventrally from the tympanic surface to an internal

41

ridge (Fig. 8) which leads to the mesal surface of the tympanal sac. Two com- pletely separate lamellae form the tympanal sac wall. Integumentary folds possi- bly formed from the metathoracic epimera overlap the tympanic surfaces and form presumed accessory tympani (Fig. 7, A.t).

The tympanic organ structures of E. lignosellus agree with the general pyraloid described above.

Explanation of Fig. 7

A.t Accessory tympanum

O Orifice for internal organs

T Tympanum

S.r Sclerotized ridge

S.o Scolophophorous organ

S.l.b Sclerotized longitudinal band

43

7. -External anterior view of the first abdominal segment of the lesser cornstalk borer moth illustrating the tympanic organs on the excised abdomen

44

Tympanum

Ridge on tympanal sac wall

Tympanal sac Scolopophorous organ

.5

Fig. 8-lnternal lateral view of the right tympanic organ of the

lesser cornstalk borer with the lateral wall of the tympanal sac removed

BEHAVIORAL STUDIES

Mating Cage Conditions

Workers used various mating cage conditions. Luginbil! and Ainsiie (19] 7) used glass lantern chimneys of unspecified size in mating and oviposition studies with single pairs. They stated that fed moths lived longer than starved, but did not distinguish between the 2 groups in their data. Dupree (1965) mated pairs of moths in 30 x 100 mm shell vials with about 30.0 cc/moth, and fed them honey diluted with 1 part water adding sodium benzoate to prevent spoilage. Leuck (1966) retained an un- specified number of moths in a 1 cu ft polyethylene covered mating cage, and fed them 10% honey-water. Calvo (1966) retained 35 moths of each sex in a screen cage with 113.8 cc/moth. No mating occurred unless there were 5 moths of each sex per cu ft. Stone (1968) used 10 moths of each sex per mating-oviposition cage with 30.3 cc/moth. Both Calvo and Stone fed moths 2% sucrose solution.

The purpose of this experiment was to find a convenient cage size with accept- able top material and cage placement for mating behavior studies, and to determine if fed and unfed moths mated with equal frequency.

Materials and Methods

Vials of 4- and 40-dr were tested as mating cages. In each of ten 4-dr vials, 1 pair of 1 -day-old (0-24 hr) moths and a cotton wad were placed. The cotton wads

45

46 were left dry in 5 vials and saturated with 2% sucrose solution in 5 vials. Satu- rated cotton wads were resaturated when nearly dry. All vials were stoppered with cotton plugs, placed horizontally, and held for 6 days. Further tests with fed moths in 4-dr vials involved 6 pairs of 1-day-old moths for 5 days, 4 pairs of 2-day-oId moths for 7 days, and 4 pairs of 3-day-old moths for 7 days.

In the 40-dr vials, mating frequency of fed versus unfed moths was com- pared. In addition, screen versus cellucotton tops and vertical versus horizontal vial placement were tested concurrently using a series of 176 vials. The influ- ence of water versus the 2% sucrose solution on mating frequency was not tested. In each of the 176 vials, 1 pair of 2-day-old moths was held 4 days. In 88 of these vials, the cotton wad was saturated with 2% sucrose solution, in the other 83 vials they were left dry. Forty-four of each set of 88 were closed with squares of 14 x 18 mesh/in fiberglas screen held in place with rubber bands, while the other 44 v/ere closed with squares of celiucotton 1/2 rnm thick held in place with rubber bands. Lastly, each set of 44 vials was again divided with 22 held verti- cally and 22 held horizontally.

At the end of each test, females were dissected for spermatcphores.

Results and Discussion

No mating occurred in 4-dr vials, where available space was 7.2 cc/moth. Under fed versus unfed conditions, 1 fed female, 3 unfed males, and 1 unfed female died.

Mating occurred in the 40-dr vials, where available space was 29.5 cc/moth (Table 4). Chi square analysis of the multiple factor test indicated no significant difference at the 1% level among position, cage top material, and fed versus

47

Tabie 4. -Cage conditions and spermatophores passed by 2-day-old fed and unfed lesser cornstalk borer adults, tested for 4 days in 40-dr vials, 1 pair per vial, 22 replicates per test.

Conditions

No. moths dead

Cages without mating

Cages

with 1 or

mere sprmts.

Multiple

matings

sprmts. cages

Total

sprmts. passed

Fed (2%

sucrose)

Screen top,

0

10

12

2

2

20

vertical

4

2

Screen top,

l$ma(2)b

10

12

2

5

19

horizontal

3

1

Cellu. top,

0

6

16

2

4

27

vertical

3

4

2

1

Cellu. top,

0

8

14

2

5

22

horizontal

4

1

Screen top,

8 66

vertical

19(0)'

Screen top,

10 66

horizontal

Cellu. top,

6dc5

vertical

Cellu. top,

5 66

horizontal

Unfed (No sucrose) 12 10

10

12

mates attached when cage dismantled number of spermatophores accepted

12

16

10

2 2

2 1

12

12

16

48 unfed moths for a single mating per pair. Fed moths had highly significantly more multiple matings than unfed moths. Twenty-nine unfed males died while no fed males died during the tests.

Forty-dr vials were thus considered adequate for mating studies. Moths should be given sugar solution to avoid death within 4 days if the moths are retained for several days, or if multiple matings are desirable.

Mating Behavior

The mating behavior of the lesser cornstalk borer has not been reported in the literature. Luginbill and Ainslie (1917) assumed mating occurred the 2nd night after emergence. Leuck (1966) stated that moths were most active in the field after dark in still air with low humidity at temperatures exceeding 80 F. He felt these conditions were optimum for mating and oviposition.

A. kuehniella and P. interpunctella females "call" prior to mating, that is, the abdomen is bent dorsally between the wings and the ovipositor is alternately protruded and retracted. Calling by P. interpunctella females is not correlated with egg development since it occurs before and after full- sized eggs are pre- sent, and after all eggs are laid (Norris, 1932).

Richards and Thomson (1932) reported mating behavior in a general discussion of the genera Ephestia, Anagcsta, and Plodia, but made no reference to behavior of a given species. Receptive females begin calling with the apical half of the abdomen bent dorsally between the wings. The ovipositor is alternately protruded and retracted. A male begins fluttering around the female which does not respond or runs away. Eventually the female stops, the male faces her head to head and bends his abdomen dorsally and anteriorly to grip the female abdomen tip. Quickly

49 the pair twists around and assumes an end to end copulatory position.

Williams (193S) stated that the A. kuehniella female calls with the wings spread and the abdomen curved upward until mating occurs. Females are quiet (=stationary?) when calling. The male moves about vibrating his wings until he meets a female. He then curls his abdomen towards her and couples.

Schwink (1953) reported that A. kuehniella and P. interpunctella males re- spond to their respective females with long-lasting whirrings. Whirs lasting 40 sec to several minutes are separated by short pauses and occur many times within several hours.

Brindley (1930) stated that copulation of A. kuehniella occurs after midnight the day of emergence, and lasts 4-6 hr. Norris (1932) reported that copulatory duration of A. kuehniella is 3-4 hr, and 1-1 1/2 hr for P. interpunctella. Williams (1938) reported that copulation lasts 3-5 hr for A. kuehniella.

The purpose of this experiment was to observe pair formation and courtship of E. lignosellus, time of coupling, duration of copulation, uncoupling of mates, and post copulatory activity. In addition the following factors were observed: number of spermatophores passed per mating; and egg development and plccement of full- sized eggs in the reproductive tract of mated and unmated females.

Materials and Methods

Pairs of 1 -day-old moths were placed in each of 5 clear plastic containers, 12 1/4 cm x 17 1/4 cm x 6 cm deep. Cellucotton squares, 3 mm thick and 4 cm on a side, were folded in 3rds, saturated with 2% sucrose solution, and placed in the left back corner of the cage floors. The cage mouths were covered with 2-mm- thick cellucotton held to the uppermost cage perimeters with rubber bands to permit

50 maximum visibility. Cages were set on platforms composed of 2 containers, iden- tical to the cage, stacked on top of each other and separated from other platforms by 28 cm. One ft behind each cage was a 7 1/2 watt red light with a reflector which illuminated the cages at about 1 ft-candle. The main lights were turned out and the red lights turned on at 8:00 PM.

The laboratory temperature during experimentation was 45.5 4- C both nights. The 1st night, the relative humidity control was turned to capacity at 8:00 PM and turned off at midnight. Before the experiment, humidify was 46% RH, at midnight 70 4- 4%, and at 8:00 AM 46 + 5% RH. The 2nd night, humidity was 46 4- 5%RH.

Five pairs of moths were observed each of 2 nights, from 8:00 PM to 1 1:00 PM at 15 min intervals, and continuously from 11:00 PM to 6:40 AM the 1st night, and from 1 1:00 PM to 5:20 AM the 2nd night. Thereafter, observations were made every 5-40 min until 8:06 AM the 1st night and 6:45 AM the 2nd night. During breaks between the final observations, females were dissected for spermatophores and for observations of egg development and placement in the reproductive system. Males were dissected and the color of the 1st secretory area of the primary simplex recorded; these results are reported in the section above dealing with primary sim- plex and spermatophore color.

Results and Discussion

Until midnight the moths remained still or ran and/or flew about the cage with no seeming orientation to each other. If a pair met, they avoided each other by turning aside or dropping to the cage floor. Females were generally more active than males up to midnight, in contrast to daylight hours when the reverse is true,

51

as seen in mating-oviposition cages in the culture room and in handling moths in other experiments.

In the description below, "calling" by females refers to a posture in which the abdominal tip is thrust between and above the wings, and the ovipositor is inter- mittently protruded and retracted. "Whirring" by males refers to the wings raised vertically over the thoracic dorsum, and forming a blur describing arcs of an esti- mated 30°. At the same time, the abdomen is raised dorsal ly with the tip between the wings, except immediately before attempted coupling.

The female initiates pair formation by calling as she remains stationary on the horizontal lower surface of the cellucotton cage top or on the vertical cage side. The male begins vibrating his antennae up and down asynchronously, makes a circle in place, and approaches the female with his wings slightly parted. If the male approaches the female from behind, he flails her abdomen tip with his antennae, the female makes a half circle in place, and the moths flail each others' antennae head to head. If on the other hand the male approaches the female head to head, the moths flail each others' antennae. The male then whirs his wings. The female may make no, 1 , or several circles in place. The moths continue flailing each others' antennae if the female does not circle, or the male flails her body with his antennae if she circles. During this time the male continues bursts of whirring, separated by brief pauses. With the female facing the male, the male continuously whirs his wings as he curls his raised abdomen with claspers extended towards the female, quickly twists toward her right or left, and strikes at her abdomen tip. If coupling is successful, the male stops whirring and pivots in a half circle as the female pivots slightly placing the body axes in a straight line, end to end, flat to

52

the surface. If the pair fails to couple, the female continues calling while the male pauses, the moths flail each others1 antennae, and the male whirs continuous- ly as he strikes again. This procedure is repeated 3-5 times until coupling occurs or the female walks away. After coupling and pivoting to the end-to-end position, the pair remains stationary and oppressed to the surface.

Uncoupling begins with the mates gradually raising the abdomens to form a 130-140 angle to each other. The male pumps his abdomen, finally raises his wings several times, and sometimes vibrates them briefly. The female may pump her abdomen as she grasps the surface. The pair may turn a half circle end back again while pulling alternately, or the male may turn about 40 and then back again. The male abdomen may be bent into a vertical "S" shape during this process or remain straight but raised vertically. The mates ultimately uncouple either from a straight line position or at a 140 angle to each other laterally.

After uncoupling, the mates tend to be active for 5-10 min moving about the cage and feeding, but finally settle down for the rest of the night.

Eight of the 10 pairs mated. Calling occurred from 1:15 AM to 6:40 AM the 1st night, and from 1:45 AM until continuous observation was terminated (6:45 AM) the 2nd night. Of 73 callings recorded, 19 initiated courtship, i.e., the attracted males whirred at least once next to the females. Of the 19 courtships, 7 led to coupling. In addition, 12 courtships occurred with females not observed calling immediately prior to courtship. However, half of these were in a single cage that was not observed as closely as the other cages, and calling could have occurred. In addition, courtship was sometimes initiated within minutes after calling began, and could easily be missed. Accepting these possibilities, apparently only calling

53

females attract males.

In 4 matings, females called only once during the night, from 1/2 to 5 min, and were coupled within 1-7 min after initiating calling. Two other females called intermittently over periods of 100 and 47 min in intervals lasting 2-54 min and 2-8 min, respectively, before coupling. The 1st was courted only once while the 2nd was courted 4 times, once before she was observed calling during the night, and 3 times when she called.

The pair formation and courtship behavior of E. lignosellus include activities suggestive of olfactory stimuli. Norris (1933) stated calling P. interpunctella fe- males stretch the intersegmental membrane bearing secretory tissue near the ductus bursae orifice. P. interpunctella males become highly excited in the presence of calling females, but never become sexually excited in the presence of non-calling females (Norris, 1933; Barth, 1937). Dickens (1936) described scent scales aris- ing from glandular areas on the 8th abdominal segment of A. kuehniella, E. cautella, E. elutella, and F\ interpunctella. Females of all 4 species had glandular inter- segmental folds between the 8th and 9th segments. E. cautella females also have 2 internal odoriferous glands which open into the oviduct near the genital pore. Gotz (1951) stated female calling in Lepidoptera exposes glands secreting sex phero- mones. Evidence does not contradict that E. lignosellus males court and couple with females that are calling. Barth (1937) stated glands of E. elutella males se- crete an odorous substance that increases female excitement in copulation. Vibra- tion of the wings disperses the odor. Courting E. lignosellus males whir with the abdomen tip, with claspers extended, held between the wings.

Coupling of E. lignosellus occurred from 1:40 AM to 6:40 AM with 7 of the 8

54 couplings occurring by 4:40 AM. Average time in copulo was 102 min (range 81- 134) including 5-10 min for uncoupling.

In unmared females, the membranous portions of the corpus bursae wall were pressed together and no visible fluid was present within the bursae. All mated females had a greenish fluid in the corpus bursae anterior to the spermatophore. Not more than 1 spermatophore was passed per mating.

Among mated females, 3 had full-sized eggs in the common and lateral ovi- ducts, 4 had full-sized eggs in the lateral oviducts but not in the common oviduct, and 1 had only immature eggs, located in the ovarioles. The 2 unmaied females had full-sized eggs in the lateral oviducts but not in the common oviduct. It appears E. lignosellus females, like P. interpunctella (Norris, 1932) mate without regard to egg development.

Influence of Additional Females on Male Mating Frequency The presence of receptive moths was an important factor in mating studies. Mating cage sizes used by Luginbill and Ainslie (1917), Dupree (1965), Leuck (1966), Calvo (1966) and Stone were summarized in the preceding experiment. Mating occurred in all cages except 4-dr vials used by Stone. Pairs of moths mated in 40-dr vials, but to assure that receptive females were present for test males in other experiments, I decided that greater female numbers per cage might be desirable. The purpose of this experiment was to determine if additional females per male influenced mating frequency.

Materials and Methods

Tv/o-day-old males and females were caged in 40-dr vials for 1 day using

55 the following numbers of individuals, male:females, with the space available per moth in each case: 1:1, 73.5 cc; 1:2, 49.0 cc; 1:3, 36.8 cc; 1:4, 25.4 cc. Equal numbers of each ratio were run on a given day until the ratios were replicated 100 times. At the end of each test, females were dissected for spcrmatophores.

Results and Discussion

Males mated with approximately equal frequency at all 4 ratios: 1:1, 60%; 1:2, 61%, 1:3, 75%; 1:4, 70%. Chi square analysis indicated no significant differences at the 5% level. Either the lack of successful mating is principally attributable to the male or else additional females resulted in inhibiting factors approximately equal to the increased probability that at least 1 female would be receptive. Not more than 1 spermatophore was passed per night per male.

Influence of Age on Mating There is much variation in the Lepidoptera as to age of mating. Richards and Thomson (1932) found that moths of the genera Ephestia and Plodia adults are ready to mate soon after emergence, almost as soon as the wings are dry. The corn earworm, H. zea, never copulates the 1st night after emergence or on the emergence night, and all copulations occur between the 2nd and 7th complete nights after emergence (Callahan, 1958a). The cabbage looper, Trichoplusia ni (Mubner), most frequently mates the 2nd and 3rd nights after emergence. Males never mate on the 1st night after emergence, but a small percentage (7%) of fe- males mated the 1st night (Shorey, 1964). However, Henneberry and Kishaba (1967) reported male cabbage loopers mated infrequently the 1st night after emer- gence and most frequently the 3rd and 4th nights after emergence. The pink

56

boilworm, Pectinophora gossypiella (Saunders), mates most frequently at ages 5-6 days (Ouye etal., 1964). The oriental fruit moth, Grapholitha molesta (Busck), mates within 24 hr of emergence, and males mate daily during the 1st 7 days after emergence (Dustan, 1964). The granulate cutworm moth female, Feltia subterranea (F.), mates most frequently the 3rd night after eclosion (Cline, 1967).

The purpose of this experiment was to determine the influence of age on mat- ing of 1-6-day-old lesser cornstalk borer males and females.

Materials and Methods

Moths 1-6 days old were tested at 1 maie:4 females and vice versa for 1 day. All combinations of ages and both sex ratios were replicated 5 times each, making a total of 360 test cages. At the end of each test, females were dissected for spermatophores.

Results and Discussion

The data were pooled as indicated in Fig. 9. The overall average for the experiment was computed since mating frequency under the 4 conditions plotted in Fig. 9 was essentially the same (average 61%, range 60-62%). Mating frequency of each age group was compared with the overall average using a 2-failed "t" test.

None of the 24 mating frequencies were significantly different from the over- all average at the 1% level. Thus the lesser cornstalk borer mates equally well at age 1-6 days under the above conditions.

Influence of Male Antennectomy on Mating Defhier (1953), Schneider (1964), and Jacobson (1965) include many re- ferences establishing the antennae of insects as the principal site of olfactory

0 O

1 Ov

57

D

2 o

0) I

O) o

O ^

E g>

.9 9

o Q

^o

LO

1

^r

oo

CM

1

1

-

1 1 1 1 1 1

% s.

o "5

o >o

n*

4)

-o i

S Ot E O

J E

o

o 2 o -o

£ s

6u!4DW %

58 receptors. This may explain why male moths deprived of their antennae or having antennae coated with various substances either do not mate or mate infrequently.

The purpose of this experiment was to determine the influence of bilateral antennectomy of the male on mating.

Materials and Methods

Two 2-day-old females were caged with two 2-day-old males handled in 1 of 3 ways. Group A males were caged untreated. Group B males were knocked down by a 5 sec exposure to COo and the left meso- and right metathorccic legs were excised between the thorax and the coxae. Group C males were also knock- ed down with CO2 as in Group B and both antennae were excised between the head and the scape. All excisions were done with the aid of a microscope.

Groups were run concurrently and replicated 25 times. Cages were retained 2 days.

Results and Discussion

In Group A, 47 spermatophores were passed, in Group B, 43, and in Group C, 1.

Complete bilateral antennectomy inhibits mating of E. lignosellus males. Based on behavior of other Lepidoptera, this is possibly due to removal of olfactory receptors which trigger pair formation, courtship, and mating on reception of the female sex arrractant.

Longevity of Virgin and Mated Moths, Sperrnatophore Passage and Acceptance, and Fecundity

The purpose of this research was to determine the longevity of virgin and mated moths, male and female mating frequency, the number of eggs laid per

59 mated female, and the temporal oviposition pattern during the total oviposition period of mated females.

Materials and Methods

Twenty-five each virgin males and females were retained for life. Newly emerged moths were placed singly in 40-dr vials and assigned an identification number. Four to 8 moths of the same sex were caged as available on a given date. Males were caged on 5 days, every 3rd day, and females were caged on 4 days, 2, 6, and 5 days apart.

Newly emerged moths used concurrently for longevity, mating frequency, and fecundity records were placed in 40-dr vials and assigned an identification number. Three 1- to 3-day-old virgin moths of the opposite sex were placed in each vial and were replaced daily for the life of the retained moths. As the re- tained moths died, they were replaced until 25 of each sex were tested. Dead retained mated females were dissected for spermatophores and retained eggs. A moth was considered dead when it failed to move appendages or pump the abdomen when gently probed. Females caged with single retained males were dissected for spermatophcres when replaced by virgin females.

The research was conducted from January to March, 1967. On 10 February, cotton wads were replaced by cellucotton wads in the 40-dr vials, since older moths tended to entangle themselves in the cotton fibers. To statistically examine the influence of this change, using analysis of variance, all retained mated moths dying prior to the change and exposed to no more than 6 days to cellucotton wads were assigned to group 1. Males with identification numbers 1-14 (excluding male no. 11 which was exposed to cellucotton wads for 11 days) and females no. 1-6

60 (excluding female no. 4 which was exposed to cotton wads for 7 days) were in group 1. All other moths were in group 2 except male no. 11 and female no. 4.

Since nearly all virgin moths survived beyond 10 February, moths were assign- ed to groups based on the dates they were initiated in the experiment. Longevity, spermatophores passed or accepted, total, viable, and sterile eggs laid, number of eggs retained at death, length of oviposition period, and longevity alone were checked statistically using correlation coefficients for mated and virgin moths, respectively.

Eggs laid by retained mated females on the cellucotton tops, vial sides and bottoms, were set aside for 24 hr before counting fertile and sterile eggs. Fertile eggs turn from cream white to red, while sterile eggs remain cream colored or turn red at one end only.

Results and Discussion

Differences in longevity and fecundity when compared with other workers (Tables 5 end 7) may result from rearing history, summarized under rearing proce- dures above, meihods of handling adults, and genetic differences. Luginbill and Ainslie (1917), Dupree (1965), and Leuck (1966) retained adults in unregulated rooms or ouidoor screened insecfaries, while Calvo (1966) maintained adults at constant temperatures and humidities. Sanchez (1960) worked with laboratory in- sects at unspecified conditions, except for a few observations discussed below. King et al. (1961) did not indicate what conditions were involved with his data. At the time of experimentation, my colony had passed thru 7 generations. Thus the genetic pool may have changed from that of the original stock that Calvo (1966) used and influenced the results.

61

Tcble 5. -Longevity of lesser cornstalk borer adults.

Reference

Sex

Sample Size

Fed

Days life

and State

Mean4-SE

Range

Median

Stone. Fla.

ma<$

25

2% sucrose

24.2-1-1.5

13-46

24

vbd

25

»

42.44-1.7

25-64

42

m 2

25

ii

18.14-1.7

12-31

17

v 2

25

n

37.6+1.8

22-55

35

Luginbill &

<5

7

some fed sugar

12.1+0.5

7-18

10

Ainslie. 1917.

sirup

Fla. &S.C.

2

7

ii

12.7+1.8

5-18

13

Sanchez. 1960. 6* 12 ? 7.5+0.3 4-12 6.5

Texas 2 7 ? 7.1+0.5 4-16 6

KingetaL 6 & 2 ? ? 8 4-19

1961. Texas

Dupree. 1965. rncJ 17c(1957) dilute honey & 11.4 Ga. mo* (1958) sodium benzoate 17.9

m2 1 7C ( 1 957) m2 (1958)

Leuck. 1966. v <5 ? 10% honey

Ga. m$ ? "

v 2 ?

11.4

2-23

17.9

11-23

14.5

3-26

19.5

7-33

22.2+1.3

3-29

10.3+0.7

4-16

21.4+3.6

10-31

a mated

b

virgin c Dupree used at least 17 pairs in 1957-1958 combined.

62

Longevity of individual mated males and females are shown in Figs. 10 and 11, respectively. Males exposed to cotton wads lived an average of 4.0 days shorter and females lived 2.5 days shorter than moths exposed to cellucotton wads. How- ever, these differences were not statistically significant.

Mated males lived an average of 12.7 days (range 5-23) after passing the last spermatophore (Fig. 10). Males no. 17 and 20 are not included in the average since male no. 17 may have died prematurely in copulo and male no. 20 failed to pass a spermatophore. Mated females lived an average of 4.7 days (range 1-13, median 4) after the last oviposition day. Virgin females lived roughly twice as long as mated females, thus agreeing with Leuck's data (1966) (Table 5).

Callahan (1958a) concluded that once a corn earworm moth mates, it be- comes less active than a virgin moth and hence lives longer on the average than a virgin. However, virgins held in holders for life lived longer than mated moths. He acknowledged his conclusion did not seem to hold for E. kuehniella (Zeller). The adults do not ordinarily feed and virgins possibly live longer by absorbing retained eggs.

Norris (1933) indicated that unfed virgin E. kuehniella females lived as long as mated females fed sugaF solution. Perhaps virgin femaies might live longer than mated females if fed, as was the case for E„ lignosellus. Feeding versus starvation is probably the decisive factor in E. lignosellus longevity, not degrees of activity at least in males, as seen in 29 deaths of mated unfed males in the mating cage conditions experiment versus no deaths of mated fed males.

Spermatophore passage and acceptance results are shown and compared with other species in Table 6. Figures 10 and 1 1 show spermatophore passage patterns.

63

Table 6.-Spermatophore passage and acceptance during the lifetime of various Lepidoptera

Individuals

Reference

Scientific and common name

Sex

Mean + SE

Range

per mating cage

66 : 99

Shorey et al.

Trichoplusia ni

9.

2.0

0-6

?

1962.

Cabbage looper

Shorey. 1964.

ii

6

2.0

0-10

1:2

Dustan. 1964.

Grapholitha molesta Oriental fruit moth

2

1.5

1-4

20:20 & 25:25

Ouye et al.

Pectincphora gossypiella

<5

4.2

0-10

l:3a

1965.

Pink boolv/orm

2

2.3

0-8

6:lab

Cline. 1967.

Feltia subterranea Granulate cutworm moth

6*

4.9

0-8

1:3

Henneberry &

Trichoplusia ni

9

1.2-1.4

?

3:3

Kishaba. 1967.°

Hughes. 1967.

Phthorimaea operculella

6

4.24-0.3

1-10

1:2

Potato tuber moth

9

2.64-0.2

1-6

2:1

Stone

Elasmopalpus lignosellus

6

7.2+0.8

0-14

1:3

Lesser cornstalk borer

2

1.7+0.2

1-3

3:1

Three virgin moths age 2-5 days, and virgins no more than 2 days old replacing dead moths, were added every 3-4 days for life of test moths.

When 75:25 66 :22 were caged, comparable results were obtained.

c The authors were checking temperature effects concurrently.

64

J8qmn^| uojjoojjijuspi 3|dvV

65

no

CM

£0

ID S LAID

1 354 (24)

_co

"CN

co

DAY EGGS LA DAY NO EGG

2

29) 1 7)2

8(3) 1 294 (34) 1

"o

00

CN

0 "SS

-o o

CN CN

CO ■«*

"*

I

(

CN

Z n^T

IT) CO

5

O

(N^ (N N N

465 (16 459 (0)

413 (3)

00

o lO

LT)

CN

JtN

6^ ^t CO CO

to _ S

"<t CO lO On

l\ 00 ^~ LO CO CO -3-

"*

?

7y

j

Z

^

I

3!

- _. _. ^n cS^2

^

•O

CO^ (> N q (J CO O

i

f

A

CO LO «N

CO ^f ""* CO

V

*

/?

■■■t

CN CO "*

7.

i.

CN

4

'A

g

^

£

A

V

o

X

/,

1

V

V

1

!

!

1

/ ,

I

/ /

i

\

j

!

\

J

I

:

I

1

£

co o

CM

o_ o

N. O (Nin N O LO

O Tf "* CO

jsqain^ uo^DOijijuspi S|dlu3j

66 Figure 12 indicates the total number of spermatophores passed by 25 males per day.

Mated males dying prior to replacement of cotton wads with cellucotton wads passed an average of 3 spermatophores fewer than males after replacement, but the difference was not statistically significant.

No more than 1 spermatophore was passed per day per male except for male no. 16 which passed 1 spermatophore to each of 2 females the 1st day of caging.

Authors included in Table 6 used various methods to determine spermatophore passage end acceptance during moth life span. Shorey (1964), Cine (1967), Hughes (1967), and Stone replaced virgins of the opposite sex daily for life of test moths. Shorey et al. (1962), Dustan (1964), and Henneberry and Kishaba (1967) caged moths at various ratios for life of females with no daily replacement of virgin males. Ouye et al. (1965) initiated studies with 3 virgin females per male and 6 virgin males per female to determine potential mating frequency of males and fe- males caged for life. Three more virgins were added every 3-4 days during the test moths1 lives, and dead moths were replaced by virgins to assure receptive moths of the opposite sex were present.

The average reproductive life of E. lignosellus males, counting from day 1 to the day the last spermatophore was passed (male no. 20 was not included since it passed no spermatophore) was 10.2 days (range 3-18, median 11) (Fig. 10). Within 3 days, a!! males except male no. 20 had mated at least once. In 5 days 49% of all spermatophores were passed, in 14 days 99% (Fig. 12).

The lesser cornstaik borer showed no significant correlation between male mating frequency (spermatophores passed) and longevity. In contrast, Shorey (1964) reported the principal factor limiting copulation frequency of T. ni males was longevity.

67

Fig. 12. -Total number of spermatophores passed per day by 25 lesser cornstalk borer males, number of males after day 13 as indicated

a 24 live males in sample

b 23

c 22

d 21

e 20

68

Shorey (1964) speculated the female may be the limiting partner, determining the average mating frequency in a population having an equai sex ratio. The lesser cornstalk borer female is the limiting partner, since males passed an average of 7.2 4- 0.8 spermatophores during a lifetime when caged daily with 3 virgin fe- males, while females accepted only 1.7 4- 0.2 spermatophore under comparable conditions.

Male no. 17 and female no. 8 remained continuously coupled to mates for 3 nights and 2 days before dying. In contrast, male no. 25 coupled on day 5 and remained coupled with 1 female until day 6, when it uncoupled and mated with another female. On day 7, it coupled again with 1 female thru day 8 when it un- coupled. The observations indicated an E. lignosellus male can disengage after prolonged coupling. Shorey (1964) and Callahan and Chapin (1960) reported that T. ni and H. zea remaining coupled during the day were unable to disengage and died coupled. Hughes (1967) mentioned mating pairs of P. operculella unable to sepcrate. Luginbill and Ainslie (1917) reported a caged pair of lesser cornstalk borer moths found in copulo unable to uncouple.

Dissections of prolonged coupled moths, in this experiment and in mating- oviposition cages used in colony maintanence, showed the cornutus, the chitinous tooth on the everted endophalus, was inserted into the bursa copulatrix and bent at a right angle to the endophalus where it entered the bursa, thus preventing re- traction of the endophalus thru the ductus copulatrix. In some cases, a malformed spermatophore collum and corpus entangled the endophalus and cornutus within the bursa and ductus copulatrix.

Table 7 compares fecundity results with those of other workers. Fig. 13

69

Table 7. -Fecundity of the lesser cornstalk borer

References and states

Sample size ($9)

Mean 4- SE

Eggs laid/female

Range

Stone. Fla. Luginbill & Ainslie.

1917. Fla. &S.C. KingetaL 1961.

Texas. Dupree. 1965.

Ga. Leuck. 1966.

Ga. Calvo. 1966.

Fla.

25

35

419.5+ 14.7 192

124

67

293-562 91-342

/(1957)

123.9

11-261

7a(1958)

61

5-221

?

125.7+20.5

2-314

Dupree used 17 55 in 195/- 1958 combined.

70 illustrates the average number and standard error of eggs laid per day. No signifi- cant correlations were found among longevity, spermatophores accepted, total eggs laid, sterile eggs laid, and length of oviposition period. Correlation between number of eggs laid and the number of fertile eggs v/as significant at the 1% level (r=.8089).

Callahan (1958a) and Shorey (1963) found no correlation between longevity and total eggs laid for H. zea and T. ni. Shorey (1963) also found egg production increased as numbers of spermatophores increased for females laying viable eggs, but percent viability was not markedly correlated with mating frequency.

In the discussion below, day refers to time of pairing, but oviposition day refers to a day counting from the 1st day eggs were laid by a particular female or by females. Once eggs are laid, even days without additional egg laying are counted as "oviposition days" within the oviposition period. This happened only 4 times (Fig. 11).

Of the eggs laid by 25 females, 56% were laid by the 4th oviposition day, or 48% by the 4th day.

The average oviposition period for 25 females, counting from the 1st through the last oviposition day was 11.8 oviposition days (range 7-19, median 10) or 14.6 days (range 8-23, median 12) counting from the 1st day of caging through the last oviposition day. Females delaying oviposition no more than 1 day aver- aged 11.5 oviposition days (range 7-19), while 5 females delaying oviposition more than 1 day averaged 13.0 oviposition days (range 10-17). Dupree (1965) found that the oviposition period was 7U8 days (range 1-18) 1 year and 4.1 days (range 1-9) the following year. Luginbill and Ainslie (1917) recorded 5 females

71

10 12 14 16

Oviposition Days

Fig. 13. -Average numbers of eggs laid per day by 25 lesser cornstalk borer females, number of females affer day 12 as indicated. Standard errors shown with horizontal lines

a

22

b

18

c

17

d

13

e

12

f

9

femal

males in sample

72

oviposited an average of 10.4 oviposition days (range 7-14).

Leuck (1966) staled that caged females oviposited nightly all their lives, but females in the work reported here lived an average of 4.7 days (range 1-13, median 4) after the last oviposition day. Dupree (1965) stated oviposition usually occurred on alternating days, rarely on consecutive days. Only female no. 2 laid larger egg numbers every other day thru day 9, with differences of 50-70 eggs on suc- cessive days. Perhaps Dupree's moths reflected temperature effects in ihe outdoor insectary, as he mentioned the average minimum and maximum temperatures during experimentation were 66.6 and 86.4 F in 1957 and 66.8 and 88.2 F in 1958. Luginbill and Ainslie (1917) stated oviposition did not occur when the temperature "fell much blow 80 F," but did not state clearly under what condition moths were kept. Sanchez (1960) stated field collected adults maintained at 65° F continued ovipositing, but adults kept at 35 F were inactive. Hov/ever, he did not study oviposition patterns. Perhaps these discrepancies in responses to temperature re- flect differing genetic strains.

The variation in numbers of eggs laid on oviposition day 1 (range 2-135, median 86) might result from varying times of mating and/or varying rates of sperm passage from the bursa copulatrix to the recepfaculum seminis.

Table 3 summarizes variations from basic oviposition patterns, and includes only females showing at least 2 of the variations listed. All females laying more than the average percent sterile eggs of total eggs laid are included (population average 5.5%, range 0-30.4%). Four of 5 females delaying oviposition more than 1 day, 5 of 8 females retaining more than 10 eggs (population average 8.7, range 1-26), and 3 of 7 females ovipositing in daily numbers differing from the usual

73

Table 8. -Lesser cornstalk borer females showing 2 or more variations from basic population oviposition patterns.

Female Ave. % Delayed ovipo- Retained 10 Irregular daily

no. sterile eggs sition (days) eggs at death oviposition pattern

+ 15

11

26 +

19 +

21

14

30%

-

22

29%

-

21

16%

5

19

13%

-

4

12%

5

11

8%

3

13

7%

7

Laid 2 fertile eggs on day 1.

74 daily decreasing pattern (Fig. 13) are included. Only females no. 21 and 22 laid more than the average total number of eggs. Females no. 4 and 19 represent the 2 lowest fecundity records obtained.

The data indicate females laying more than the average percent sterile eggs of all eggs laid tend to show other variations. Seven females not included in Table 8 showed only 1 variation of the 4 Usted.

Nineteen females began ovipositing on day 2. Female no. 22 laid eggs the 1st day of caging, while 5 females delayed oviposition more than 1 day (Fig. 1 1). A. kuehniella females underwent periods of quiescence after mating, usually 12 to 24 hr. During this time the sperm passed from the bursa copulatrix to the recepta- culum seminis. Then oviposition began, lasting to within the last day or 2 of life (Norris, 1933). If this is the case in E. lignosellus, then one could assume the 19 females probably mated on day 1 and were ready to oviposit on day 2. Female no. 22 must have mated on day 1, as the 2 eggs laid were fertile. The 5 females delaying oviposition may have mated the day before they initiated oviposition or perhaps they indicated a wide range of sperm passage rate from the bursa to the glandula receptaculum.

Thirteen females laid decreasing numbers of eggs from oviposition day 1, dis- regarding increases of less than 10 eggs in production on 2 successive days. If oviposition day 1 is disregarded due to the variation in egg numbers laid, then 18 females laid decreasing numbers of eggs daily. Norris (1933) found A. kuehniella females laid the greatest number of eggs during the 1st 2 days and then decreased production gradually until the last day or 2 of life, when 1, 2, or no eggs were laid. This agrees with lesser cornstalk borer egg production, except that E.

75 lignosellus females live longer on the average after the last oviposition day.

Of females differing from the daily decreasing oviposition pattern, 1 tended to oviposit on alternate days (female no, 2 discussed above), 1 reached peak production on oviposition day 3, and another on oviposition day 4. Four females reached a 2nd peak production (at least 15 more eggs laid' than on the previous oviposition day) after the 1st 2 oviposition days 2 on oviposition cay 4, 1 on 5, and 1 on 6.

A. kuehniella females laid sterile eggs at any point in life (Norris, 1933). All gradations in fertilization reduction occurred and oviposition of no viable eggs was associated with the absence of sperm from the receptaculum seminis of mated females or with the presence of small quantities of sperm, much smaller than in normally mated females- When spermatozoa were present, they were less violent- ly motile than usual, and in some cases they were motionless, perhaps due to re- tarded spermatogenesis, which also might cause the male to pass reduced quantities of sperm. Altho the above factors were not checked in my research, they might have influenced sterile egg production.

Twenty-two females laid less than 10% sterile eggs of all eggs laid on ovi- position day 1. The 3 females laying more than 10% laid 20, 49, and 66%. Two females laid sterile eggs every oviposition day (females no. 21 and 22), while 2 females Icid no sterile eggs during the entire oviposition period (females no. 6 and 23).

Concerning spermatophores accepted by females listed in Table 8, 4 females delaying oviposition accepted 1 spermatophore each. A 5th moth delaying ovi- position accepted 2 and oviposited in a daily pattern of decreasing numbers of

76 eggs as shewn in Fig. 13. Assuming no pcrthenogenesb occurred, all 5 females mated on or by oviposition day 1, since each laid some fertile eggs on oviposition day 1. The other 3 females in Table 8 accepted 1, 2, and 3 spermatophores (fe- males no. 19, 14, and 22, respectively). Females no. 4, 11, 19, and 21 tended to lay increasing percentages of sterile eggs daily as fewer eggs were laid. Per- haps the sperm supply was becoming exhausted with time.

The 3 of 25 females accepting 3 spermatophores retained 14-19 eggs at death. Five other females (Table 8) (4 accepted 1 spermatophore, 1 accepted 2) retained more than 10 eggs at death. Correlation between the number of spermatophores accepted and the number of eggs retained at death was significant at the 5% but not at the 1% level (r=.5006).

Of females ovipositing daily eggs numbers varying from the basic curve (Fig. 13), 3 females accepted 1 spermatophore and 4 females accepted 2.

No record was kept of how many eggs were laid by virgin females retained for life, nor were the eggs retained for hatch. However, other workers hove not reported that parthenogenesis occurs among the Phycitidae.

Time of Oviposition Few workers have reported the time of oviposition of the lesser cornstalk borer. Luginbill and Ainslie (1917) stated that oviposition of caged females be- gan shortly after dusk and continued until the early morning hours. The majority of eggs were laid during the forepart of the night. No eggs were laid diurnally or in bright light at night. Leuck (1966) reported that caged femalss oviposited shortly after dark and throughout the night.

77

Materials and Methods

Paper sheers were placed on screen tops of 3 mating-oviposition cages as de- scribed under rearing techniques. The sheets were replaced every 4 hr starting at 3 PM 1 day and ending at 7 PM the following day. To determine if oviposifion occurred during the hour before the lights fumed off (at 8 PM), sheets v/ere left on the cages from 7-8 PM at the end of the experiment. All sheets were set aside for at least 30 hr. The eggs were then counted with the aid of a microscope.

Results and Discussion

All 3 populations oviposited over 90% of all eggs laid from 7-1 1 PM (92, 95, 98%). From 11 PM to 3 AM, the 3 populations oviposited 8, 3, and 2% of all eggs laid, respectively. The remaining eggs were laid between 3 AM and 7 AM, except for 1 sterile egg laid between 7-8 PM on the 2nd day. Thus moths oviposit primarily during the 1st 3 hr of total darkness.

Response of Adults to Sound

Sound reception by moths has attracted considerable attention in recent years. The tympanic organs of noctuid moths are sensitive to sounds ranging from 3-240 kc/sec with maximum sensitivity between 15-60 kc/sec (Roeder and Treat, 1957). Tympanic nerve preparations defect bat cries at a distance of 30 m or more (Roeder and Treat, 1960).

Insectivorous bats use ultrasonic cries to echolocate night flying insects (Griffin, 1953; Griffin and Novick, 1955; Novick, 1965). Roeder and Treat (1960) found that many free flying moths perform evasive behavior in the pre- sence of bats. The same is true when moths are subjected to an artificial

78 approximation of bat cries (Roeder, 1962; Agee, 1967). The intensity of the sound stimulus is directly related to the type of moth response (Roeder, 1964); diving re- sponses are most prevalent around 75-85 db, while turning-away responses are most prevalant around 45-55 db. No evidence indicates tympanate moths can dis- tinguish differences in sound frequency (Roeder, 1966). It was concluded that the evasive behavior had a selective advantage and that probably the major function of moth tympanic organs was to warn night flying moths of approaching bats (Roeder and Treat, 1960).

Several workers examined practical application of moth response to sound. Belton and Kemps ter (1962) broadcast ultrasonic sound at 50 kHz and obtained more than 50% reduction of sweet corn infestation by the European corn borer, Ostrinia nubilalis (Hubner). Treat (1962) captured more than twice as many tym- panate moths in silent light traps than in light traps broadcasting recorded ultra- sonic bat cries. Agee (1967) attempted to reduce oviposition of bollworm moths, H. zea, and tobacco budworm moths, Heliothis virescens (F.), in cotton fields with ultrasonic sound. He felt the negative results obtcined were due to equip- ment failure during the moths' most active periods. Payne and Shorey (1968) found that pulsed ultrasonic sounds, especially at high intensities, reduced ovi- position by the cabbage looper moth, T. ni, on lettuce and broccoli crops.

The tympanic organs might have other auditory or proprioceptive functions unconcerned with bat detection (Trect, 1955), such as echo-location (Roeder and Treat, 1957). However, Treat (1955) suggested diurnal Lepidoptera possessing tympanic organs might have recently acquired the diurnal habit, and the organs have persisted without survival value. On the other hand, perhaps the diurna!

79 fliers also fly at night when an auditory sense might be of more benefit. He men- tioned the typically diurnal butterflies have a poorly developed auditory sense, if it exists at all.

Roeder and Treat (1960) inferred that most moths with known auditory organs are medium sized (10-40 mm wing span). Few of these moths escaped attack by bats but many escaped capture.

In addition to the tympanic organs, Lepidoptera have several other organs or structures which may assist in sound perception (Haskell, 1961). These include: (A) Johnston's organ in the 2nd antennal segment; (3) the subgenual organs, generally found in the proximal region of the tibiae of all legs; (C) chordoronal sensillia, scattered about the body; and (D) hair sensillae, scattered over the body but especially on the thorax and abdomen. All of these structures respond to 10 kc/sec or less so far as is known among the Insecta, but little research has been done with the Lepidoptera. Functions attributed to the tympanic organs might in fact be carried out by the above structures in combination with each other and/or with the tympanic organs, since no one has reported bat avoidance by deafened moths.

Further, adult Lepidoptera themselves produce sound by various means, but little is known about their behavioral significance (Haskell, 1961; Alexander, 1967). Haskell (1961) catagorized the principal types of sound producing mech- anisms into 2 groups. The 1st group includes sounds produced by products of some usual moth activity, as the ultrasonic sound of 15 kc/sec and possibly higher pro- duced in the flight of Prodenia eridania. Roeder and Treat (1957) suggested the sound might be associated with precopulatory behcvior. Shorey (1964) found that

80 bilateral tympanectomy of both sexes of T. ni possibly reduced but did not prevent successful copulation.

The 2nd major group includes several mechanisms such as factional mecha- nisms, vibrating membranes, and mechanisms directly involving air movement (Haskell, 1961). These mechanisms include: (A) scraping raised fore and hind wing veins together producing frequencies up to 14 kc/sec, as in the Peacock Butterfly (Nymphaiis jo); (B) rubbing wing ridges with some part of the leg, as in many noctuid moths; (C) clicking of wing membranes which pop in and cut when the wings are knocked together, as in Hecatesia; (D) rubbing a ribbed and a pegged wall of an abdominal cavity together, as in certain Lymantriid male moths; (E) forcing air thru the proboscis by means of pharynx pumping with the epipharynx interrupting air flow, as in Acherontia atropos; and (F) possibly forcing air thru spiracles, as in Arcfia caja.

Perhaps some of the above mechanisms include clues to communication be- tween the sexes involving sound perception by the tympanic organs, but as Alex- ander (1967) indicates, this possibility has scarcely been investigated. In addition, the tympanic organs sense natural sounds other than bat cries, as rustling leaves and cricket chirps. Other functions could be served by the tympanic organs opart from bat detection, but no evidence of the importance of such perception is avail- able as yet (Roeder and Treat, 1961a, 1961b).

The purpose of this experiment was to determine if E. lignosellus adults re- spond to sonic and ultrasonic sound.

Materials and Methods

A cylindrical cage of fiber glass screen and clear plastic, 6.5 cm high x 2.5

81 cm diam, was used to cage individual test moths. The cage top and bottom peri- meters were plastic rings 1.5 cm and 0.5 cm wide, respectively. The screen was sewn together along the side with thread and glued to the 2 rings. A small piece of fitted fiber glass screen formed the bottom. The cage with a moth was inverted into a small plastic dish with a fitted cellucotton disk saturated with 2% sucrose solution.

Preliminary tests indicated 66-100% of moths tested nocturnally (10:30 PM to 1:30 AM) at 3-16 kHz with 3.3-20 volts of amplitude remained quiet throughout the tests. Other moths moved about the cage and/or stroked their antennae, while still others extended their abdomens with tone bursts and contracted the abdomens to the normal position during silence after a tone burst. This latter activity was used as the criteria for a positive response in the following tests since it was re- peatable at 20-60 kHz.

Five each 2-day-old males and females were tested for behavioral response from 2-4 PM and another set of 5 males and 5 females of the same age from 9:45- 1 1:00 PM. The cage was set 46 cm directly in front of a Dukane lonovac^ Duk-5 speaker with a power supply modified for extended frequency response. Pure tones from 20-60 kHz in 10 kHz increments were produced with a Hewlett-Packard audio oscillator model 200 CD. Three 1/2 sec tone bursts were separated by 4

CR)

sec using a General Radi<j tone burst generator type 1396-A timed by a Hewlett- Packard^ audio oscillator model 201 C. At least 10 sec separated a tone burst triplet from the next.

Voltages of amplitude scale settings were determined with an oscilloscope as: 20 scale setting =1.1 volts; 30 = 1.9 volts; 40 = 3.3 volts; 50 = 6.4 volts; 60 =

82

13.5 volte. A General Radio sound level meter type 1551-B was used to measure intensity on the A scale at 46 cm from the speaker. At 10 kHz, the following amplitude scale settings were recorded in decibells: 10 = 54 db; 20 = 61 db; 30 =

66 db; 40 = 71 db; 50 = 76 db; 60 = 80 db. These sound levels correspond to the

CD

above voltages. The loncvac tweeter was found to be linecr within + 2,5 db

between 10 and 60 kHz. Consequently the voltage readings should be convertible to sound levels with the same scale for all frequencies.

Moths were subjected to various frequencies and amplitudes. In the 1st test, frequencies were held constant in the following order: 40, 30, 50, and 40 kHz. In each frequency test, amplitude dial settings were varied in the following order: 60, 40, 20, 30, and 50.

In the 2nd test, amplitude was held constant at full setting (20 volts) as fre- quency was varied in the following order: 20, 30, 40, 50, 60, 40, 20, 30, 50, and 60 kHz.

The cage was lighted with about 23 ft-candles during the afternoon tests. During the evening teste, a 7 1/2 watt red light 7.5 cm distant illuminated the cage with about 2-3 ft-candles. The room temperature was 27 4- 1 C.

Results and Discussion

In general, moths responded to high and low frequencies at high amplitudes (Fig. 14). Females responded essentially the same during day and night, but males were more responsive at night.

The test of bilateral tympanectomy on sound response was attempted but abandoned due to moth size and delicacy and the tympanic organ position. The wings, thorax and/or abdomen were usually damaged during the operation. Unlike

83 nocfuid tympanic organs, those of E. lignosellus are located on the 1st abdominal segment and are covered by the metathoracic coxae. In addition, a fan of elon- gated scales originating on the abdominal pedicel obscures the tympanal surface. The operation was performed by anesthetizing a moth with CCU, placing it on its back, pressing with a pin on the sclerotized rim surrounding the tympanic organs, and thrusting the pin point dorsally and posteriorly simultaneously. The operation was essentially done in the blind. Often the tympanic membrane and scolopo- phorous organ were still intact in moths checked later to determine operational success.

E. lignosellus is responsive to sound, but whether this is due to reception by the tympcnic organs was not determined. The function of the tympanic organs in E. ligncseilus is unknown.

84

? 5r

Z I 3

11

-o-o

0 4 8 12 16

Amplitude (volts)

30 kHz

12 16

Amplitude (volts) 40 kHz

0 4 8 12 16 Amplitude (volts) 50 kHz

Diurnal response

Amplitude (volts) 30 kHz

Amplitude (volts) 40 kHz

Amplitude (volts) 50 kHz

Nocturnal response

iOr

o c

. o

£ 9r

I 2

Z

o

E

0 20 40 60

kHz Amplitude 20 volts

Diurnal and nocturnal response combined

o $ response -o C* response

Fig. 14. -Diurnal and nocturnal response of lesser cornstalk borer adults to various amplitudes and frequencies.

SUMMARY

Need for basic research on the reproductive biology of the lesser cornstalk borer, Elasmopalpus lignosellus (Zeller) (Lepidoptera: Phycitidce) prompted this research. The borer reportedly attacks 62 host plants representing 14 plant families, but host records may be incorrect due to similar feeding habits of other species.

A rearing technique was developed. Sixty to 80 pupae were obtained per 100 eggs. Tote I time from egg to adult was 24-28 days. The colony was reared for 34 months or approximately 32 generations. Ninety-five percent of pupae obtained were normally formed. Lightly sclerotized aberrations occurred on the venter of 5% of pupae obtained. Approximately 92% and 78% emergence of normal adults was obtained from normal and aberrant pupae respectively.

The male and female reproductive systems, including spermatophore morphology and position in the bursa, were studied and compared with other Lepidoptera. Color- less fluid in the 1st secretory area of the primary simplex of the male indicated a mating (=spermatophore transfer) less than 24 hr previously. Virgin 1-6-day-old males had translucent yellow simplex fluid, and males that mated 2-5 days pre- viously had transparent yellow simplex fluid. Within 24 hr after mating, simplex fluid in 3-day-old mated males changed from transparent colorless to transparent yellow. Color of spermatophores representing 3 successive matings was clear trans- parent. Thus 1st matings were indistinguishable from subsequent matings on the

85

86 basis of spermatophore color. Females had no full-sized eggs af emergence, but might have them present in the calyx, lateral oviducts, ana/or common oviduct as well as in the ovarioles within 1-2 days after emergence.

The abdominal tympanic organs were studied and compared with the general pyraloid description. The tympani were ventrally and anteriorly located on the 1st abdominal segment.

Mating occurred in 40-dr vials but not in 4-dr vials. In the 40-dr vials no significant differences in mating success occurred in respect to cage position, cage top materials, and fed versus unfed moths for a single mating per pair. Fed moths were more likely to mate again than unfed moths.

Mating behavior was observed, including pair formation, courtship, time of coupling, duration of copulation, uncoupling of mates, and post copulatory activity. Pair formation and courtship behavior included activities suggestive of olfactory stimuli. Mating occurred from 1:40 AM to 6:40 AM. Males mated with approxi- mately equal frequency when caged for 1 day with 1, 2, 3, or 4 females. One- to 6-doy-old moths mated equally well. Complete bilateral antennectomy of males inhibited mating.

Virgin males lived 42.4 4- 1.7 days (mean 4- standard error), mated males 24.2 4- 1 .5 days, virgin females 37.6 4- 1.8 days, and mated females 18. 1 4- 1.7 days. Males passed 7.2 4- 0.8 sperrnatophores each and females accepted 1.7 4- 0.2 sperma- tophores each in a lifetime. There was no significant correlation between longev- ity and sperrnatophores passed. Females laid 419.5 4- 14.7 eggs each of which 5.5% were sterile and retained 8 eggs at death. Oviposition began the 2nd day of caging with males, and decreasing numbers of eggs were laid daily throughout the

87 oviposiMon period. Females oviposited 48% of all eggs laid by the 4th day of caging with males. Females laying more than the average percent of sterile eggs tended to delay oviposition more than 1 day, to retain more than 10 eggs at death, and/or oviposit non-decreasing daily numbers of eggs during the oviposition period. There was no significant correlation among longevity, spermatophores accepted, total eggs laid, sterile eggs laid, and length of oviposition period. Correlation between number of eggs laid and number of fertile eggs was significant at the 1% level (r=8089). Correlation between the number of spermatophores accepted and the number of eggs retained at death was significant at the 5% level (r=5006). Moths responded to sound of high and low frequencies at high amplitudes. Males were more responsive nocturnally than diumally, but females showed little differentia! response.

LITERATURE CITED

Agee, H. R. 1967. Response of acoustic sense cell of the bollworm and tobacco bud worm to ultrasound. J. Econ. Entomol. 60:366-369.

Alexander, R. D. 1967. Acoustical communication in arthropods. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 12:495-526.

Arthur, B. W., and F. S. Arant. 1956. Control of soil insects atlcching peanuts. J. Econ. Entomol. 49:68-71.

Ash, C. R., and F. F. Bibby. 1957. Field test of soil treatmenis for prevention of stGnd destruction of grain sorghum by lesser cornstalk borer. Arizona Fertilizer, Inc. Crop Comments. 11:2-4.

Bailey, L. H. 1949. Manual of cultivated plants. Macmillan Co. , New York. 1116 p.

Bartiett, B. R., and C. H. Martin. 1945. A chemical method of freeing cocoons of Macrocenirus ancylivorus from tuber moth cocoons. J. Econ. Entomol. 38:

Berth, R. 1937. Herkunft, Wirkung und Eigenschaften des weiblic'r.en Sexuclduft- sfoffes einiger Pyraliden. Zool. Jahrb. , Abt. Zool. Physiol. Tiere. 58: 297-329.

Belton, P., and R. Kempster. 1962. A field test on the use of sound to repel the European corn borer. Entomol. Exp. Appl. 5: 231-288.

Berger, R. S. 1963. Laboiatory techniques for rearing Heliothis species on arti- ficial medium. U.S. Dep. Agr., ARS 33-34. 4 p^

[Bissell, T. L.J 1945. Lesser cornstalk borer, p. 63-64. In Ga. Agr. Exp. Sfa. 57th annual report for 1944-1945. ~~

[Bissell, T. L.J 1946. Lesser cornstalk borer on cowpeo:., p. 82-84. In Ga. Agr. Exp. Sic. 58th. annuol report for 1945-1946.

Bissell, T. L. , and M. Dupree. 1947. Vegetable insect pests. Georgia Agr. Exp. Sta. Bull. 254: 7-8.

88

89

Bcurgogne, J. 1951. Ordre des Lepidopferes, p. 174-448. ]n Traite de Zoologie, vol. 10. Masson et Cie, Paris.

Brindley, T. A. 1930. The growth and development of Ephestia kuehniella Zeller (Lepidoptera) and Triboiium confusum Duval (Coleoptera) under controlled conditions of temperature and relative humidity. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Arner. 23: 741-757.

Callahan, P. S. 1958a. Behavior of the imago of the corn earworm, Heliothis zea (Eoddie), with special reference to emergence and reproduction. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Amer. 51: 271-283.

Callahan, P. 5. 1958b. Serial morphology as a technique for determination of reproductive patterns in the corn earworm, Heliothis zea (Boddie). Ann. Enrornoi. Soc. Amer. 51:413-428.

Caliahar,, P. S., and T. Cascio. 1963. Histology of the reproductive tracts and rrcnsmission of sperm in the corn earworm, Heliothis zea. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Amer. 56: 535-556.

Callahan, P. S., and J. 3. Chopin. 1960. Morphology of the reproductive systems and mating in two representative members of the family Noctuidae, Pseudaletia unipuncta and Periodroma margaritosa, with comparison to Heliothis zea. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Amer.~~53:"763-782.

Calvo, J. R. 1966. The lesser cornstalk borer Elasmopaipus lignosellus (Zeller) and its control. Unpublished Ph. D. Dissertation, Univ. "ofTTa. 70 p.

Chittenden, F. H. 1900. Some insects injurious to garden crops. U.S. Dep. Agr. , Div. Entomol. Bull. 23 n.s. 92 p.

Chittenden, F. H. 1903a. Some insects recently injurious to truck crops, p. 113- 120. jnU.S. Dep. Agr., Div. Entomol. Bull. 40.

Chittenden, F. H. 1903b. The principal injurious insects in 1902, p. 726-733. jn U.S. Dep. Agr. Yearbook for 1902.

Cholodkovsky, N. 1884. Ober die Hoden der Lepidopferen. Zool.Anz. 7: 564-568.

Cline, L. D. 1967. Mating and reproduction of the granulate cutworm moth, Feltia subterranea (Fabricius). Unpublished Masters Thesis, Univ. of Fla. 49 p. ~

Cov.-art, F. F. , and A. H. Dempsey. 1949. Pimiento production in Georgia. Ga. Agr. Exp. Sta. Bull. 259: 17.

Cunningham, W. H., Jr., D. R. King, and S. C. Langley. 1959. Insecticidal con- trol of the lesser cornstalk borer on peanuts. J. Econ. EntomoL 52: 329-330.

90

Dekle, G. W. 1965. A lepidoptercus larva on peach, p. 34-36. In Florida Dep. Agr., Div. Plant Industry, 25th Biennial Rpt.

Dempsey, A. H., and B. B. Brcntley. 1953. Pimento production in Georgia. Ga. Agr. Exp. Sta. Bull. 227. 27 p.

Dethier, V. G. 1953. Chemoreception, p. 544-576. In Insect Physiology. K. D. Roeder ed. , John Wiley and Son;, New York.

Dickens, G. R. 1936. The scent glands of certain Phycitidae (Lepidoptera). Trans, Entomol. Soc. London. 1936: 331-362.

Dupree, M. 1964, Insecticidal and cultural control of the lesser cornstalk borer. Ga. Agri. Exp. Sta. Mimeo. Ser. N. S. 197. 21 p.

Dupree, M. 1965. Observations on the life history of the lesser cornstalk borer. J. Econ. Entomol. 58: 1156-1157.

Dustan, G. G. 1964. Mating behavior of the oriental fruit moth, Grcpholitha molesta (Busck) (Lepidoptera: Olethreutidae). Can. Entomol. 96: 1087-7093.

Eggers, F. 1919. Das thoracale bitympanale Organ einer Grouppe der Lepidoptera Heterocera. Zool. Jahrb. , Anat. (Jena). 41: 273-376.

Eggers, F. 1925. Versuche uber das Gehor der Noctuiden. Z. vergl. Physiol. 2: 297-314,

Eggers, F. 1928. Die stiftfuhrenden Sinnesorgane. Zool. Bausteine. 2: 1-351.

Eidmann, H. 1931. Morphologische und physiologische Untersuchungen am

weiblichen Genitalapparat der Lepidopteren. II. Physiol. Teil. Z. angew. Entomol. 18:57-112.

Fernald, M. L. 1950. Gray's manual of botany, 8th Edition. American Book Co., New York. 1632 p.

Forbes, S. A. 1905. A monograph of insect injuries to indian corn. III. State Entomol. 23rd Rpt. 273 p.

Gotz, B. 1951. Die Sexualduftstoffe an Lepidopteren. Experientia. 7:406-418.

Griffin, D. R. 1953. Bat sounds under natural conditions with evidence for echolocation of insect prey. J. Exp. Zool. 123:435-466.

Griffin, D. R. , and A. Novick. 1955. Acoustic orientation of neotropical bats. J. Exper. Zoo!. 130: 251-299.

91

Harding, J. A. I960. Control of the lesser cornstalk borer attacking peanuts. J. Econ. EntcmoL 53: 664-667.

Haskell, P. T. 1961. Insect sounds. Quadrangle Boob, Inc., Chicago. 189 p.

Heinrich, C. 1956. American moths of the subfamily Phycitinae. U.S. Nat. Mus. Bull. 207. 581 p.

Henneberry, T. J., and A. N. Kishaba. 1967. Mating and oviposition of the cabbage looper in the laboratory. J. Econ. Entomol. 60: 692-696.

Hill, A. F. 1937. The nomenclature of the cultivated sorghums. Harvard Bot. Leaf I. 4: 173-180.

Hitchcock, A. S. 1951. Manual of the grasses of the United States. Revised Edition. U.S. Dep. Agr. Misc. Publ. 200. 1051 p.

Howard, L. O. 1900. The principal injurious insects of the year 1899, p. 745- 748. In U.S. Dep. Agr. Yearbook for 1899.

Hughes, I. W. 1967. Reproductive behavior of the potato tuberworm, Phthorimaea operculella (Zeller), (Lepidoptera: Gelechiidae) and the effect of the cherno- sterilant metepa. Unpublished Ph. D. Dissertation, Univ. of Fla. 95 p.

Isely, D., and F. E. Miner. 1944. The lesser cornstalk borer, a pest of fall beans. J. Kansas Entomol. Soc. 17: 51-57.

Jacobson, M. 1965. Insect sex attractants. Interscience Publishers, New York. 154 p.

Kelsheimer, E. G. 1955. The lesser cornstalk borer* Fla. Grower and Rancher. 63: 20, 36.

Kennel, J., and F. Eggers. 1933. Die abdominalen Tyrnpanalorgane der Lepidopteren. Zool. Jahrb., Anat. (Jena). 57: 1-104.

King, D. R., J. A. Harding, and B. C. Langley. 1961. Peanut insects in Texas. Tex. Agr. Exp. Sta. MP-550. 14 p.

Kiriakoff, S. G. 1963. The Tympanic structures of the Lepidoptera and the taxonomy of the order. J. Lepidopferists' Soc 17: 1-6.

Leuck, D. B. 1966. Biology of the lesser cornstalk borer in South Georgia. J. Econ, Entomol. 59:7^7-801.

Luginbill, P., and G. G. Ainslie. 1917. The lesser corn stalk-borer. U.S. Dep. Agr. Bull. 539: 27 p.

92

Lyle, C. 1927. The lesser cornstalk borer. Store Plant Board of Mississippi, Quart. Bull. 7: 2-3.

Musgrave, A. J. 1937. Histology of the male and female reproductive organs of Ephestia kuhniella. Proc. Zool. Socs London. 1937: 337-364.

Norris, M. J. 1932. Contributions towards the study of insect fertility. I. The structure and operation of the reproductive organs of the genera Ephestia and Plodia (Lepio'optera, Phycitidae). Proc. Zool. Soc. London. 1932: 595-61 1.

Norris, M. J. 1933. Contributions towards the study of insect fertility. II. Experiments on the factors influencing fertility in Ephestia kuhniella Z. (Lepidoptera, Phycitidae). Proc. Zool. Soc. London. 1933: 903-934.

Novick, A. 1965. Echolocation of flying insects by the bat, Chilonycteris psilotis. Biol. Bull. 128: 297-314. "

Ouye, M. T., H. M. Graham, C. A. Richmond, and D. F. Martin. 1964. Mat- ing studies of the pink bollworm. J. Econ. Entomol. 57: 222-225.

Ouye, M. T., R. S. Garcia, H. M. Graham, and D. F. Martin. 1965. Mating

studies on the pink bollworm, Pectinophora gossypiella (Lepidoptera: Gelechiidae), based on presence of spermatophores. Ann. EntcmoL Soc. Amer. 58: 880-832.

Payne, To L., and H. H. Shorey. 1968. Pulsed ultrasonic sound for control of oviposition by the cabbage looper moth. J. Econ. Entomol. 61: 3-7.

Petersen, W. 1907. Ober die Spermatophoren der Schmetterlinge. Z. wiss. ZooL 88: 117-130.

Reynolds, H. T., L. D. Anderson, and L. A. Andres. 1959. Cultural and chemical control of the lesser cornstalk borer in southern California. J. Econ. Entomol . 52: 63-66.

Richards, O. W. , and W. S. Thomson. 1932. A contribution to the study of the genera Ephestia, gn. (including Strymax, Dyar), and Plodia, gn. (Lepidoptera Phycitidae), with notes on parasites of the larvae. Trans. Entomol. Soc. London. 80: 169-250.

Riley, C. V. 1882a. New insects injurious to agriculture, p. 272-273. In Proc. Amer. Asso. Adv. Sci. 30th meeting.

Riley, C. V. 1882b. New insects injurious to agriculture. Amer. Natur. 16: 152-153.

Roeder, K. D. 1962. The behavior of free-flying moths in the presence of artifi- cial ultrasonic pulses. Anim. Behav. 10: 300-304.

93

. 1964. Aspects of the noctuid tympanic organ having significance in the "avoidance of bars. J. Insect Physiol. 10: 529-546.

. 1966. Interneurons of the thoracic nerve cord activated by tympanic

nerve fibers in noctuid moths. J. Insect Physiol. 12: 1227-1244.

Roeder, K. D., and A. E. Treat. 1957. Ultrasonic reception by the tympanic organ of noctuid moths. J. Exp. Zool . 134: 127-157.

. 1960. The acoustic detection of bats by moths. Proc. Intern. Congr. Entomol., 11th. 3:8-11.

. 1961a. The detection and evasion of bats by moths. Amer. Sci. 49:

135-149.

. 1961b. The reception of bat cries by the tympanic organ of noctuid moths, p. 545-560. In Sensory communication. W. A. Rosenblith, [ed.] John Wiley and Sons, Inc., New York.

Sanchez, L. O. 1960. The biology and control of the lesser cornstalk borer, Elasmopalpus lignosellus Zeller. Unpublished Ph. D. Dissertation, Texas Agr. and Mech. College. 74 p.

Schneider, D. 1964. Insect antennae. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 9: 103-122.

Schwink, I. 1953. Uber den Sexualduftstoff der Pyraliden. Z. vergl. Physiol. 35: 167-174.

Shorey, H. H. 1963. The biology of Trichoplusia ni (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae). II. Factors affecting adult fecundity and longevity. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Amer. 56: 476-480.

. 1964. Sex pheromones of noctuid moths. II. Mating behavior of

Trichoplusia ni (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae) with special reference to the role of the sex pheromone. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Amer. 57: 371-377.

Shorey, H. H. , L. A. Andres, and R. L. Hale, Jr. 1962. The biology of Trichoplusia ni (Lepidoptera: Noctuidae). I. Life history and behavior Ann. Entomol. Soc. Amer. 55: 591-597.

Snow, J. W., and T. C. Carlysle. 1967. A characteristic indicating the mating status of male fall arrnyworm moths. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Amer. 60: 1071-1074.

Stahl, C. F. 1930. The lesser cornstalk borer (Elasmopalpus lignosellus Zell.) attacking strawberry plants. J. Econ. Entomol. 23: 166.

Stitz, H. 1901. Der Genitalapparat der Mikrolepidopteren. II. Der weibliche Genitalapparat. Zool. Jahrb. Abt. Anat. 15: 385-434.

94

Stone, K. J. 1968, Reproductive biology of the lesser cornstalk borer. I. Rearing technique. J. Econ. Entomol. In Press.

Treat, A. E. 1955. The response to sound in certain Lepidoptera. Ann. Entomol. Soc. Amer. 48: 272-284.

Treat, A. E. 1962. Comparative moth captures by an ultrasonic and a silent light trap. Ann. Entomol. Soc0 Amer. 55: 716-720.

U.S. Department of Agriculture. 1952-1968. Coop. Econ. Insect Rpt.

Vorhies, C. T., and L. P. Wehrle. 1946. Pest problems of the small garden, Ariz. Agr. Exp. Sta. Bull. 203: 33-34.

Walton, R. R., R. S. Matlock, and J. P. Boyd. 1964. Effect of the lesser corn- stalk borer on peanuts in Oklahoma. Okla. Agr. Exp. Sta. Processed Ser. P-474. 10 p.

Webster, F. M. 1906. The principal injurious insects of 1905, p. 628-636. In U.S. Dep. Agr. Yearbook for 1905. ~

Williams, J. L. 1938. The mating of Ephestia kuehniella Zeller and its results. Entomol. News 49: 104-107, 121-126^

Williams, J. L. 1941. The relations of the spermatophore to the female repro- ductive ducts in Lepidoptera. Entomol. News 42: 60-65.

Wilson, J. W. , and E. G. Kelsheimer. 1955. Insects and their control. Fla. Agr. Exp. Sta. Bull. 557. 28 p.

BIOGRAPHICAL SKETCH

Kari Johnson Stone was born in Petoskey, Michigan, on 7 February 1935. He received his primary education at Central Grade School and his secondary edu- cation at Petoskey High School, graduating in June 1953. He entered the Uni- versity of Michigan in the fall of 1953, and received the degree of Bachelor of Science in June 1957.

He was admitted to the Graduate School of the University of Michigan in September 1957, and received the degree of Masters of Science in Biology in June 1959.

He was appointed to the position of Scientific Technician with the Arctic Re- search Laboratory, operated by the University of Alaska under contract with the Office of Naval Research in September 1959, and held the position until he was cppcinred Administrative Assistant to the Director, Arctic Research Laboratory, in August 1961.

He was admitted to the Graduate School of the University of Florida in September 1962. He was appointed to the position of Laboratory Technician with the Florida Department of Agriculture, Division of Plant Industry in July 1963 and held the position until June 1965. He was appointed to the position of Research Associate with the University cf Florida, Department of Entomology, in July 1965.

95

96 Karl J. Stone is married to the former Margaret Ann Brand, and is the father of one child. He a member of the American Museum of New York, Society of Systematic Zoology, Entomological Society of America, and the Florida Entomo- logical Society.

This dissertation was prepared under the direction of the chairman of the candidate's supervisory committee and has been approved by all members of that committee. It was submitted to the Dean of the College of Agriculture and to the Graduate Council, and was approved as partial fulfillment of the require- ments for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy.

December, 1968

^V

/ft

/ Dean, College of Agriculture

Dean, Graduate School

Supervisory Committee:

Chairman

^C'VvnfUn C ch^b-^*

n^°-

«8&

mi

^H