•*€> STUDIES OF INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS AND RATS BY W. E. CASTLE and SEW ALL WRIGHT Published by the Carnegie Institution of Washington WASHINGTON, 1916 ('akxegie Institution of Washington, Publication No. 241 Paper No. 26 of the Station for Experimental Evolution at Cold Spring Harbor, New York From the Laboratory of Genetics of the Bussey Institution Copies of this Book were first issued SEP 20 1916 43i PRESS OF GIBSON HROTHERS. INC. WASHINGTON CONTENTS. Part I. — An Expedition to the Home of the Guinea-pig and Some Breeding Experiments with Material there Obtained. Bt W. E. Castle. page. Introduction 3 Some observations on guinea-pigs in Peru 5 Hybridization experiments with Cavia cutleri 8 Life history of C. cutleri 8 Crosses of C. cutleri males with guinea-pig females 13 Color inheritance among the F2 hybrids 13 (a) Cross 9 albino (race B) X d" C. cutleri 13 (6) Cross 9 albino (race C) X cf C. cutleri 14 (c) Cross 9 brown-eyed cream (race C) X cf C. cutleri 16 (d) Results from (b) and (c) combined 16 (e) Intensity and dilution among the hybrids 17 (/) Significance of the results observed 18 Hybridization experiments with a race of feral guinea-pigs from lea, Peru 20 Origin and characteristics of the lea race 20 Crosses between the lea race and guinea-pigs of race C 23 The F2 generation 24 Summary on the lea race 29 Hybridization experiments with a domesticated guinea-pig from Arequipa 31 d1 1002 and his Fi offspring 31 F2 offspring of d" 1002 33 Back-cross and other offspring of d1 1002 35 Miscellaneous matings of the descendants of cf 1002 36 Summary on the Arequipa domesticated race 41 Size inheritance in guinea-pig crosses 42 Previous work on size inheritance 42 Weights and growth curves of C. cutleri, of various guinea-pig races, and of their hybrids 43 Skeletal measurements of C. cutleri, of various races of guinea-pigs, and of their hybrids 47 The C. cutleri hybrids 48 Hybrids of the Arequipa d1 1002 52 The lea hybrids 53 Theoretical explanations of size inheritance and of "blending inheritance" in general 54 Part II. — An Intensive Study of the Inheritance of Color and of Other Coat Characters in Guinea-pigs, with Especial Reference to Graded Variations. By Sewall Wright. page. Color and its inheritance in guinea-pigs 59 Skin, fur, and eye colors of guinea-pigs 59 Color of Cavia cutleri 59 Melanin pigment 59 Primary classification of fur colors 59 Yellow group of colors Dark group of colors "0 Skin colors "1 Eye colors "2 Definitions of fur colors by Ridgway's charts 62 Definitions of eye colors Heredity of fur and eye color Color factors of guinea-pigs 63 Classification of color factors 63 iii jv CONTENTS. . iad us inheritance in gumearpiga-Gmtinued. PAGE- II, r. ditv of fur and eve color— Continued ^ ( "oior m, white fi4 Lntenatj of genera] color development "* I >ark M. J ellow color \ .in.it 1. .us of dark color 1 able of factor combinations Hereditary factora and the physiology of pigment °' :> Of experiment- Material 7, - • .mat io position Description of stocks ' 1 7 < >1 >lcmB [nberitanoe of dilution \} The red-eye factor \} I Hlul ion ™ The dilution factor • 80 Inheritance of minor variations in intensity 85 Methods and accuracy of grading 85 Variation! in intense guinea-pigs and albinos 85 Multiple allelomorphs • .' ' ' " ^ The relations of imperfect dominance, stock, and age to grades of intensity . . 87 Yariat ions of yellow 89 Varial ions of sepia 92 Variations of eye color 93 Summary 93 Inheritance of variations in the agouti pattern 94 Previous work 95 The inheritance of the agouti of C. rufescens 96 Minor variations 99 inheritance of the agouti of C. cvileri 100 Inheritance of rough fur 100 Classification 102 Previous work 102 Material 103 Problems 104 Inheritance of rough as opposed to smooth 105 Inheritance of major variations 106 Possibilities of linkage among rough and color factors 113 Summary of rough tables 115 >r variations 116 ighnees of Beries II 117 Summary 118 conclusion 119 1 data 121 tables 62 to 137 121 r '" 1 ' iMiiii: BxxmrjBB of Piebald Rats and Selection, with Observations one Coupling. By W. E. Castle. PAGE. Jed with wild rats 163-168 lection of the hooded pattern of rats 168-172 rurth' on t|1(. mutant series 173-174 n yellow rata 175-180 181-187 188-190 191-192 PART I AN EXPEDITION TO THE HOME OF THE GUINEA-PIG AND SOME BREEDING EXPERIMENTS WITH MATERIAL THERE OBTAINED By W. E. CASTLE INTRODUCTION. For several years I have been engaged in studies of heredity in guinea-pigs. In the course of these studies all the common varieties of guinea-pigs have been investigated by the method of experimental breeding and something has been learned concerning their inter- relationships and probable mode of origin. The actual origin of most of these varieties is, however, unknown, as is true also concerning most varieties of domesticated animals. One or two varieties have, however, been made synthetically in the laboratory and it is conceivable that, if we had the original wild stock to work with, from which the domesticated guinea-pig has arisen, some or all of the existing varieties might be synthesized anew and perhaps still others might be obtained, and that in this way something might be learned of the method by which new varieties arise. From considerations such as these I have for several years been seeking to obtain living specimens of the wild species which most closely resemble guinea-pigs. In 1903 I received from Campinas, Brazil, 3 wild-caught individuals referred at the time to the species Cavia aperea, but since found to agree better with the description of C. rufescens. From two of these animals young were obtained, and crosses, the results of which have been described in detail by Dr. Detlefsen (1914), were made with domesticated guinea- pigs. It may be noted that all male Fi hybrids were sterile, but that the Fi females were fertile, and that upon repeated crossing of these with male guinea-pigs, a race of fertile hybrids was at last obtained, these being, in the language of breeders, about f guinea-pig, § rufescens. From this result it seems doubtful whether C. rufescens has any close genetic relationship to the domesticated guinea-pig, although by hybridization it has been found possible to produce races (f or more guinea-pig) which have derived certain characters from a rufescens ancestor. Cavia aperea from Argentina has been crossed with the guinea-pig by Nehring (1893, 1894) in Berlin, with the production of fully fertile hybrids. This result indicates a closer relationship with the guinea-pig than C. rufescens manifests. Darwin (1876), however, did not regard aperea as the ancestor of the guinea-pig, because he found it to be infested with a different species of louse. I have not myself been able as yet to obtain specimens of C. aperea. Nehring (1889) has argued with much plausibility that Cavia cutleri of Peru is more probably the ancestor of the guinea-pig, for (1) it agrees closely with the guinea-pig in cranial characters and it occurs in a region where guinea-pigs have been for a long time kept in domestication, as is shown by the occurrence of mummified guinea-pigs which had been buried with the dead. Natu- rally I formed a strong desire to secure living specimens of C. cutleri for 3 \ tNTRODUCTION. mental study, l>ut for several years I was unable to do so. rough pondence with Professor S. I. Bailey, who was at the tin tor of the Harvard Astronomical Observatory at Arequipa, oertained thai a wild species of cavy occurred in that locality. IY< :■ • r Bailey kindly captured some of the cavies and attempted eatedly to forward them to me, but without success. The steam- ship roiiip.-inii-; n-fn>«'«l to accept them for transportation on the ground that they might lead to detention or quarantining of their vessels, .11 rodents were inspected of being carriers of bubonic plague. 1 yean of waiting and fruitless negotiation with every chance traveler to Peru with whom I came in contact, I resolved to to Peru myself and get the desired specimens. Through a grant made by the ( Sarnegie Institution of Washington I was enabled, in the fall of 191 1. to carry this resolution into effect. The < larnegie Institution of Washington and the Bussey Institution ha mther provided means for carrying out the breeding experi- dm 9Ciibed in this paper. I wish to express my gratitude to both institutions and to thank the director and other officers of the Harvard 1 ege < Observatory for hospitality and generous assistance given me at the Arequipa station. I am indebted also to Professor C. J. Brues for kindly bringing me a stock of guinea-pigs obtained by him near Lim l Peru, in 1912. SOME OBSERVATIONS ON GUINEA-PIGS IN PERU. On a midsummer day in December 1911 I arrived as a guest at the Harvard College Observatory in Arequipa, Peru, where I went in search of guinea-pigs, wild and domesticated, to be used in breeding experiments. The day after my arrival at the observatory I walked a short dis- tance up the highway through a group of adobe cabins, straw-thatched and without chimney or windows, and with a single door. On looking in at the open door of one of the cabins, I was pleased to see a domesti- cated guinea-pig of the common spotted black-and-white sort familiar to lovers of pet-stock throughout the world. In other near-by cabins I found considerable numbers of guinea-pigs were kept, in one as many as 40. They were fed on fresh-cut alfalfa or the green leaves of maize, receiving apparently no other food and no water. At the back or sides of the cabin was a sort of shelf or bench of stone used as a seat or couch, underneath which the guinea-pigs had their home. Their escape through the open door was prevented by a high lintel of stone, perhaps 15 inches (38 cm.) high, over which one has to step in entering. In these cabins were seen most of the common color varieties of guinea- pigs known to us, agouti, black, yellow, and white (albino). None of the colored individuals which I saw was self-colored ; all were spotted with white or with yellow or in both ways. The same predilection for spotting is seen in the other important native domesticated animal, the llama. I saw no llamas except such as were spotted; some were black spotted with white, but the majority were of a soft shade of buff or fawn spotted with white. The common spotted condition of our guinea-pigs is undoubtedly one of long standing; indeed it would seem that the Peruvian natives breed no other variety except such as are either white spotted or all white. The unspotted or " self-colored " varieties now kept by fanciers in Europe and America have probably been produced by selection from stock originally spotted. This is indicated by the great difficulty in securing a self-colored race entirely free from spotted individuals. Most self-colored races, even when bred for many generations from self-colored ancestors exclusively, will pro- duce an occasional individual bearing a few hairs or a patch of hairs of some other color, or of white. Among the guinea-pigs kept by the natives near Arequipa, I observed an occasional animal having a rough or rosetted coat. This variety is known to fanciers in Europe and the United States under the name Abyssinian. (See Castle, 1905.) It is said, on the authority of Geoffroy Saint-Hilaire, to have been introduced from Peru into Europe about the year 1872 in a rough-coated, long-haired individual received at the Jardin d'Acclimatation, Paris. In conformity with this account 5 6 INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. it may be said that the rough-coated long-haired variety has ever since its introduction been called by fanciers "Peruvian." I saw no long- haired individuals, cither rough-coated or smooth, among the guinea- pigs kept by the natives at Arequipa, and the short-haired rough-coated one- observed had imperfectly developed rosettes, much inferior to the best standard-bred rosetted Abyssinians of fanciers in Europe and the United States. For this reason I infer that no particular attention was given to this character in the breeding of the guinea-pigs which I saw. though this may very likely have been done in other parts of the country. But the unit-character variation which is responsible for the rosetted condition of the coat in Abyssinian guinea-pigs was plainly represented in the stocks kept by the natives in Arequipa and needed only selection to bring it up to the standards of fanciers. Eight independent mendelizing unit-character variations had been recognized as affecting the coat characters of guinea-pigs up to this time. Six of these were represented among the four or five dozen guinea-pigs which I actually saw in the cabins of natives, the other two unit characters being (1) the long-haired variation which, as already noted, is said to have been brought originally from Peru to Europe; and (2) the brown variation which first came to the notice of fanciers in England about 1900 and was certainly in existence before that time in the United States, as I can state from personal knowledge. It is uncertain whether or not this last variation had already occurred in Pent and was thence transferred to Europe, but it is certain that all the other 7 had done so, and it is very probable that this also originated in Peru. Further, a ninth wholly independent unit-character variation (presently to be described, viz, the pink-eyed variation) has made its appearance in stocks of domesticated guinea-pigs obtained by me at Arequipa in 1911 and by my colleague, Professor C. T. Brues, at Lima, in 1912. So it is clear that this variation also is widely disseminated among domesticated guinea-pigs kept by the natives in Peru and which have never been in the hands of European fanciers at all. It can be stated, therefore, with probable correctness, that the guhiea- pig has undergone in domestication more extensive variation in color and coat characters than any other mammal, and that this variation has occurred almost if not quite exclusively under the tutelage of the natives of Peru. This conclusion points either to a great antiquity of the guinea-pig :i- a domesticated animal or to more rapid evolution by unit character variation than by other natural processes. That the natives do give careful attention to the selection of animals f«»r breeding is shown by the following incident: In the cabin near the observatory, where I first saw guinea-pigs in Peru, and where I ulti- mately s. •cured two pairs of animals, one of which I brought back with me, I observed a very large individual which I desired to purchase, and though other individuals were offered me at a very reasonable price, GUINEA-PIGS IN PERU. 7 this particular one could not be had because, I was assured, he was the "padre" (sire) of the entire family. Size seemed to be the point especially emphasized in the breeding of guinea-pigs in this cabin, as would naturally be the case when the animals formed the meat-supply of the family, as they do now among the native poor of Peru and doubt- less have done since ancient times. But the chief object of my journey to Peru was the study not of the domesticated guinea-pigs of the country, but of their wild progenitors. Accordingly special efforts were made to secure specimens of the wild cavy, which Professor Bailey had found to be abundant in the locality. Once or twice, when riding along a road between irrigated fields, I had seen a cavy scurry to cover in a pile of rocks; further, I had observed droppings of the animals in the rocky wall of a cattle corral in an alfalfa field. But how to capture the animals alive was a problem which baffled immediate solution. It seemed likely that the natives would know better how to go about this than I did. Accordingly word was passed around among the near-by villages that a good price would be paid at the observatory for wild cavies, either alive or dead. Within a few hours boys began to arrive with the coveted specimens and for the next week I was kept busy preparing skins and saving bones of the animals which were received dead, or making cages and caring for such as arrived alive. In this way 11 cavies (all I could hope to transport safely) and about a dozen skins were soon secured, and preparations were made for the return journey. In due time the journey was accomplished, and with such success that three new races of guinea-pigs were added to our experimental stocks, viz, (1) a wild species, the probable ancestor of the domesticated guinea-pig, identified as Cavia cutleri Bennett; (2) a feral race from lea, probably identical with that described by Von Tschudi; (3) domesticated guinea-pigs, such as are at present kept by the natives of Peru. 8 INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. HYBRIDIZATION EXPERIMENTS WITH CAVIA CUTLERI. LIFE HISTORY OF CAVIA CUTLERI. The primary object of my journey to Peru was to secure representa- tives of the wild species of cavy, Cavia cutleri Bennett, known to exist there. Four pairs of these animals captured at Arequipa were suc- fullv installed in cages at the Bussey Institution in January 1913. One of the males soon died without leaving descendants; the other 7 animals 1 4 females and 3 males) produced offspring in captivity, which have continued to breed succesfully, though the stock has at times been seriously reduced by disease in cold weather. Three generations of descendants have been reared from the original stock of 7 animals. Together they number 100 individuals, of which 47 are males and 53 females. All are very uniform in color, size, general appearance, and behavior. Their color is a dull leaden gray-brown, well adapted to escape notice amid the arid surroundings of their native habitat. The fur is agouti- ticked and the belly light, but the yellow of the ticking and belly is so pale as to resemble a dirty white or very light cream shade. The color much paler than that of the Brazilian species, Cavia rufescens, studied by Detlefsen. The fur is also finer and softer, in which respect it ■mbles the guinea-pig. The size of C. cutleri is about the same as that of C. rufescens, and between one-third and one-half that of the guinea-pig. The maximum weight of an adult male is about 525 grams ; that of a domesticated male guinea-pig obtained in Arequipa (d*1002) is nearly three times this amount. In wildness ( 'aria cutleri is very much like C. rufescens. The animals live contentedly in small cages, 2 feet 6 inches square, but invariably retreat under their box or conceal themselves in the hay if anyone approach The extreme >avageness toward each other of individuals of Cavia cutlrri makes it difficult to rear large numbers of them in captivity. It i- seldom possible to keep more than a single pair in a cage together for any length of time. Two adult males will not live together peace- ably under any circumstances, and if two females are placed together in a cage with one male persecution of one female by the other usually follows. Even when the young are ahWed to grow up in the same cage with their parents, family dissensions are likely to arise as soon as th<- young become mature. The period of gestation (minimum interval between litters) averages 3 horterthan in guinea-pigs, being 60 to 70 days, and the number of young to a littervaries from 1 to4. Fifty-three litters bornin captivity includ tly UK) young, an average of 1.89 young to a litter. The si/e of litter occurring most frequently is 2, which has been recorded CAVIA CUTLERI. 9 Table 1. — Number and size of litters -produced by each mother, Cavia cutleri. Mother and date of her birth. Date of litter. Size of litter. Mother's age at birth of young. Days since last litter. 9 2 (caught wild) ; born March 1911 (?) 9 3 (caught wild) ; born May 1911 (?) 9 5 (caught wild) ; born Jan. 1910 (?) Mar. 5, 1913 June 28, 1913 Aug. 29, 1913 Nov. 4, 1913 May 29, 1912 Oct. 3, 1912 Dec. 26, 1912 July 5, 1913 Dec. 15, 1913 July 12, 1912 Sept. 12, 1912 Nov. 15, 1912 Jan. 22, 1913 Sept. 6, 1912 Sept. 26, 1912 Dec. 10, 1912 Feb. 17, 1913 June 30, 1913 Oct. 1, 1913 Aug. 15, 1914 Dec. 2, 1914 July 5, 1912 Sept. 4, 1912 Nov. 4, 1912 Apr. 25, 1913 June 25, 1913 Aug. 25, 1913 June 17, 1913 Oct. 16, 1913 Aug. 3, 1914 Nov. 2, 1914 Julv 26, 1913 Sept. 20, 1913 Aug. 25, 1914 Nov. 2, 1914 July 26, 1913 Nov. 1, 1913 Dec. 27, 1913 June 25, 1913 Aug. 29, 1913 June 28, 1913 Sept. 4, 1913 Nov. 4, 1913 Jan. 5, 1914 Nov. 1, 1913 Dec. 27, 1913 Mar. 1, 1914 Mar. 15, 1914 May 18, 1914 July 20, 1914 Julv 30, 1913 Dec. 8, 1913 Jan. 12, 1915 2 2 2 2 3 3 1 3 1 3 3 3 2 3 1 2 1 3 2 2 1 1 2 2 1 1 1 1 4 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 2 3 2 2 1 2 1 1 3 2 1 1 1 2 1 1 2 months 24 27 29 31 12 17 20 26 32 18 20 22 24 20 4 5 7 12 15 25 29 10 12 14 7 9 11 8 12 22 25 8 10 21 24 6 9 11 5 7 3f ! 6 4 6 4 6 8 6 8 10 9 14 14 62 67 62 64 68 69 61 61 61 61 56 69 56 65 68 62 64 64 63 9 6 (caught wild) ; born Jan. 1910 (?) 911; May 29, 1912 9 15; July 12, 1912 9 26; Sept. 6, 1912 . 9 27; Sept. 6, 1912 9 36; Oct. 3, 1912 9 42; Nov. 15, 1912 9 65 ; Jan. 22, 1913 9 66; Jan. 22, 1913 9 79; March 5, 1913 9 118; June 28, 1913 9124; June 30, 1913 9129; July 5, 1913 9 184; Sept. 4, 1913 9 224; Oct. 16, 1913 9 241 ; Nov. 4, 1913 LO INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. M times; litten of I have been recorded 18 times, litters of 3, 10 times, md .i litter of 4 once. Factors which influence size of litter are evi- dently age and state of nourishment of the mother. Table 2 shows the relation of age of mother to size of litter. Very young mothers (age •I months Of lees) have only 1 young at a birth. The females become wallv mature a1 a very early age, as do female guinea-pigs. Well- Dourifthed females may breed at 2 months of age, when they are less than half-grown, full growth not being attained until they are 12 or 13 months old Females over 4 months but under 12 months of age produce usually 1 or 2 young at a birth, rarely 3; those which are 1 or Id produce the maximum number of young, usually 2 or 3, rarely 1 or 4. After the age of 2 years the number of young again Table 2. — Relation between age of mother and size of litter, Cavia cutleri. Ag«' -f mother in month- Size of litters and number of each size. Age of mother in months. Size of litters and number of each size. 1 in litter. 2 in litter. 3 in litter. 4 in litter. 1 in litter. 2 in litter. 3 in litter. 4 in litter. 4 3 1 ■i 3 2 1 1 2 3 1 1 1 2 12 to 15 16 to 19 20 to 23 24 to 27 28 to 31 32 Total litters. . . 1 1 1 1 3 1 2 6 2 2 2 3 1 1 6. . . . : > in 11 18 24 10 1 decreases to 1 or 2. The oldest female known to have borne young 4 the original stock) had at the time been in captivity over 2 and her estimated age was 32 months. None of the females horn in captivity has given birth to young at a more advanced age than 29 months. Our records accordingly indicate that females rarely ed after fchey have attained the age of 2\ years. The duration of the breeding period in the case of males is more extended. It is prob- able that males •!<> not attain sexual maturity quite so early as females, f<»r females may breed when less than 2 months old, but we have no denoe that males can breed before they are 3 months old.1 But the capacity t.» breed once attained continues indefinitely. One male * 1 caught wild in December 1911 and estimated then to have been tenths old Lb -till siring young, more than 3 years after his capture, being, it U .--timated, nearly 4 years old. males are capable of breeding again immediately after the birth i. but if they do bo the number of young at the next birth is Dtion (o a record from his experiments which shows that a male light infusbo <.f r blood must have been sexually mature at 2J - the only record known to me of a guinea-pig male breeding when lesa CAVIA CUTLERI. 11 apt to be less, or the young will be born smaller and less fully developed (with smaller bodies and shorter hair) , and the period of gestation will be shortened, even to 56 days in extreme cases, the normal period being, as in the guinea-pig, between 60 and 70 days. (See table 4.) If the mother is well nourished and has not borne a litter recently, she is more likely to have a large litter of young. The largest litter recorded (4) was borne by a female 1 year old, which had previously had only 1 young, born 4 months earlier. The recorded date of the birth of each litter of young is given in table 1, together with the interval in days between suc- cessive litters by the same mother, except in cases where the interval is obviously greater than the ordinary period of gestation, and it is to Table 3. — Relation of size of litter and number of litters to time of year. Born in — Size of litters and number of each size. Total Utters. Total young. 1 in Utter. 2 in Utter. 3 in litter. 4 in litter. January 1 1 2 1 1 3 2 1 2 1 3 1 3 1 2 5 5 1 4 1 4 7 February March April May June Total born in first 6 months1 July 9 5 1 0 15 22 2 1 1 1 4 3 5 3 1 5 2 3 2 1 2 1 1 8 6 6 3 8 7 17 11 13 9 17 11 August September October November Total born in sec- ond 6 months2. . 9 19 9 1 38 78 1Average, 1.47 young per litter. 2Average, 2.05 young per litter. be supposed that the mother did not breed again immediately. The variation in these day intervals between litters is shown in table 4, from which it appears that the gestation period ordinarily continues from 61 to 69 days, with 63.3 days as an average. However, the periods as recorded can not be relied upon as accurate, except within limits of about 2 days, for the cages were not inspected daily, but only once or twice a week, and when young more than 24 hours old were found in a cage, the estimated age of the young may differ from the true age by 1 or 2 days. Young less than a day old are readily recog- nized as such by the condition of the umbilical cord. The 4 original wild-caught females have a somewhat better record of productiveness than their descendants reared in captivity, which indi- 12 INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. rates that laboratory conditions of close captivity are not as favorable for full growth and vigor B8 the freer life and better air of the original habitat. The4 wild-caught females produced 33 young in 14 litters, an average of 2.36 young to a litter. Their daughters or granddaughters, reared in captivity, when of like age, have produced 27 young in 13 litters, an average of 2.07 young to a litter. Too much emphasis must not be laid, however, on this difference, because productiveness depends largely on food, care, and weather con- ditions, and it is not certain that these were equally favorable for the original females and for their descendants, respectively. Table 1 shows for each mother how many litters of young she has borne, at what age she bore them, and how many young were contained in each litter. In the case of the 4 females caught wild, the age given for the mother is of course not known; the age recorded is an estimate based on the size of the mother when captured. Table 3 shows in what month each litter of young was born and what it- size was. This table brings out rather strikingly the effect of the ions and consequent character of food available upon the size and number of the litters. Taule 4. — Variation in period of gestation {interval since previous litter) in Cavia cutleri. Average, 63.3 days. Days Days between Cases. between Cases. litter.-. litters. 56 2 65 1 61 4 67 1 62 3 68 2 63 1 69 2 64 3 In the 6 months from July to December inclusive, litters were born which were conceived under summer conditions, with an abundance of green food available. It will be observed that in this half of the year the litters are numerous (38) and large (average 2.05 young to a litter). The young born in the (i months January to June inclusive were con- i under winter conditions, when the mothers were subsisting largely on a diet of dried or concentrated foods, with a limited amount of green food available. In this half of the year the litters are less numerous 15 and smaller (average size 1.47 young). uper.it i, re probably does not directly affect the result, as the mak were kept in a he.-, ted house, but purity of the air may possibly the house is much better ventilated in the warmer months. - probably the most important factor, as the condition of the mak changes promptly with change of food, even when other condi- tion- -how no ehani CROSSES OF CAVIA CUTLERI. 13 CROSSES OF CAVIA CUTLERI MALES WITH GUINEA-PIG FEMALES. Crosses have been made only between male Cavia cutleri and female guinea-pigs. The reciprocal cross was not undertaken, because the number of cutleri females on hand at any one time has been insufficient and because it seemed probable that a cross with the much larger guinea-pig would be fatal to the cutleri females because of the probable large size of the hybrid offspring. Two races of guinea-pigs were employed in the crosses, these being the purest races available, the genetic properties of which had been long and thoroughly tested. The race most extensively used may be called race C. It consisted of "brown-eyed cream" individuals or of albinos borne by brown-eyed cream parents. The results of crosses of colored and albino individuals of race C will be described separately. The other race may be called race B. It consisted of intensely black-pigmented individuals or of albinos produced by such black individuals. The results of crosses with the two sorts of individuals will be described separately. A cutleri male bred in captivity (cf 78) was mated with black females of race B, and produced 9 F: young, all colored like C. cutleri, but darker, the ticking of the fur being brick red or yellow instead of creamy white as in cutleri. Albino females of race B were mated with the same cutleri male (d"78) or with cf 114, another cutleri male reared in captivity, or else with cf 4 or cf 8, which were original cutleri males caught wild. Such matings produced 39 Fx young, all with the same (golden agouti) type of coloration as the young produced by the black mothers. Females of race C were mated only with the two wild-caught cutleri males (r?4 and c?8). The cream-colored mothers of race C produced 34 young, all golden agouti in color like the young derived from race B crosses, but much lighter. They were, however, darker in color than C. cutleri, the agouti ticking being yellow or reddish, not creamy white as in cutleri. (See plate 3.) Albino females of race C produced by the cutleri males 14 young, indistinguishable in appearance from the young produced by their cream-colored sisters. The Fi hybrids, whose total number was 96, were all vigorous and large, their adult size nearly or quite equaling that of guinea-pigs. They grew with great rapidity and have proved fully fertile inter se. In wildness and ferocity they are intermediate between the parent races. COLOR INHERITANCE AMONG THE F2 HYBRIDS. (a) CROSS 9 ALBINO (RACE B)Xc? CUTLERI. By breeding inter se certain of the F: hybrids, from the cross 9 albino, race B, X cf cutleri there has been produced a second (or F2) generation of hybrids, which number 75 individuals. As regards color, disregarding minor differences of intensity of pigmentation, these hybrids fall into three classes: golden agouti, black, and albino. Of the It INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. utia then are 13, of the blacks 15, and of the albinos 17. The DumericaJ relations ol the classes suggest a dihybrid Mendelian ratio 0| g i, which is in entire agreement with existing knowledge of odor inheritance in guinea-pigs (Castle, 1905; Sollas, 1909). C. cutleri i- evidently homozygous for all Mendelian color factors, since it breeds very true to color. Albino guinea-pigs from a black race are known to possess two independent recessive modifications from this condition, lacking both the agouti factor and the so-called color factor. As u-ds these factors, then, the wild race, cutleri, forms gametes AC, the albino forms gametes ac, and the Fx hybrids form gametes of the four types \t '. Ac, aC\ and ac. From recombination of such gametes Bhould arise in F2 zygotes as in table 5. Table 5. 1 AACC \:iCC 2 A A' !c 4 AaCc ■'>uti 1 aaCC 2 aaCc 3 black 1 AAcc 2 Aacc 3 albino 1 aacc 1 albino Th' ral kinds of albinos being similar in appearance, the expected result is 9 agouti. 3 black, 4 albino. The agreement with this expecta- tion is fairly close (see table 6). Table 6. Agouti. Black. Albino. Observed Expected 43 42.19 15 14.06 17 18.75 (b) CROSS 9 ALBINO (RACE C)Xt? CUTLERI. Ft anini.il> from the cross between an albino of race C and a cutleri male have produced 11 F2 young, which fall into 7 color classes, dis- regarding differences of intensity of pigmentation. These classes and their numerical representation among the 44 young are as follows: golden agouti, 10; black, 1: cinnamon, 8; black-eyed cream, 4; brown- ing; chocolate, t; albino, 14. (Seeplate4.) The occurrence of thee eral classes of 1", young is what previously existing knowl- ; color inheritance among guinea-pigs would have led us to expect, for it was known thai albinos of race C differed from agoutis in the same two factors as the albinos of race B, viz, the agouti factor and the color factor. In addition, the albinos of race C were known to differ from lUtifl in two other factor-, -eon respectively in chocolate and yellow The chocolate race may be considered to have arisen by a recessive modification of the black factor B, and the yellow race by a similar modification of the extension factor E. Accordingly this cross CROSSES OF CAVIA CUTLERI. 15 was supposed in advance to involve 4 independent Mendelian factors, a supposition which the observed result justifies. The factor differences in the two races are: gametes of cutleri, ABCE; of albino (race C), abce. On this hypothesis the Fi hybrids should form 16 different kinds of gam- etes, the color potentialities of which are indicated in parentheses: 1 ABCE (agouti). 7 AbcE (albino). 12 Abce (albino). 2 aBCE (black). 8 abCE (chocolate). 13 aBce (albino). 3 AbCE (cinnamon). 9 aBCe (black-eyed yellow) . 14 abCe (brown-eyed yellow) 4 ABcE (albino). 10 aBcE (albino). 15 abcE (albino). 5 ABCe (black-eyed yellow). 11 AbCe (brown-eyed yellow). 16 abce (albino). 6 ABce (albino). From this list it will be observed that 2 different gametic factorial combinations are capable of producing black-eyed yellow, and that the same is true concerning brown-eyed yellow, while 8 different com- binations contain the potentialities of albinos. From these considera- tions it follows that the F2 ratio will be peculiar, since each of the yellow classes that can be distinguished from each other (black-eyed and brown-eyed) will itself be composite, and the same will be true of the albino class. The expected classes and their proportional frequencies will accordingly be : Golden agouti 81 Black 27 Cinnamon 27 Yellow (black-eyed) 36 Yellow (brown-eyed) 12 Chocolate 9 Albino 64 A cross involving the formation of so many classes of individuals can not be expected to show very satisfactory Mendelian ratios in so small a number of offspring as 44. All the expected classes are represented, although black is represented in a single individual only. The colored individuals of race C were known in many cases to carry albinism as a recessive character. The albino gametes of such indi- viduals would, in crosses with cutleri mates, form the same kind of zygotes as the albinos of race C, which were used in the cross just described. In considering the results of such crosses, it is therefore proper to include in one category Fi animals derived from both sources. If this is done the F2 young are increased to 108, distributed as shown in table 7, the expected theoretical number in each class being shown in a parallel column. Table 7. Observed. Expected. Observed. Expected. Golden agouti Black 33 7 13 13 8 34.17 11.39 11.39 15.08 5.07 Chocolate 7 27 3.80 27.00 Black-eyed yellow .... Brown-eyed yellow. . . . 108 108.00 L6 INHl.l.MI \\« I. IN GUINEA-PIGS. (c) CROSS 9 BROWN-EYED CREAM (RACE C) X c? CUTLERI. Tl,.- ]• animals produced by crossing brown-eyed cream females of race ( « ith cvtt ri males themselves produced 132 F2 young, distributed among the ■«"»" ~ classes as the albino cross had produced (table 8). Table S. rved. I oted. Observed. Expected. ek .-■: 17 10 18 8 48 9 1 16.31 16.31 21 76 7 . 26 5 16 5.44 132 116.00 allow. . . . Brown-cQ ad yellow 1 Kcepl in regard to albinos, the result expected from this cross is the M»itf as that expected from crossing albinos of race C. Accordingly the albinos may be for the moment disregarded. The expectation as regards the colored classes of young may then be stated as shown in the column of "observed" results. The occurrence of 10 albinos in this F2 generation shows that certain of the Fj pairs were heterozygous for this character, which they obvi- ously derived from the brown-eyed cream parent, not from the cutleri parent; for the brown-eyed cream animals of race C were known in many eases to be capable of producing albinos, whereas the cutleri stock bred true Accordingly such pairs of Fx animals from this cross as pro- duced albino young should be tabulated with the hybrids produced by crossing albinos of race C with cutleri males. If this is done there remain 68 instead of 116 F2 young to be considered. Among these are 3 albino- which it is impossible to transfer to table 7, because it is not known what colored individuals were born in the same litters with them. They were born in a pen containing 2 females which had young simul- taneously, one of which was known to produce albino young, though the other did not. Omitting the 3 albinos, there remain 65 colored young, distributed as Bhown in table 9. Table 9. oted. Observed. Expected. .1 11 III 27 i- 0 1 1 i i 12 19 Brown-eyed yellow ( Ihocolate 3 2 4.06 3.05 65 65.00 (d) RESULTS FROM (b) AND (c) COMBINED. e the expectation is the Bame as regards the relative proportions al colored classes in all crosses of race C females (whether albmo or colored with cutleri males, we may legitimately combine all CROSSES OF CAVIA CUTLERI. 17 the F2 results, omitting only albinos (which have been dealt with already). When this is done we get the results shown in table 10. No class deviates from expectation enough to suggest "linkage" or " coherence" of characters involved in the cross. Table 10. Observed. Expected. Observed. Expected. Golden agouti Black Cinnamon Black-eyed yellow .... 67 18 18 23 61.59 20.53 20.53 27.37 Brown-eyed yellow. . . . Chocolate 11 9 9.13 6.85 146 146.00 (e) INTENSITY AND DILUTION AMONG THE HYBRIDS. It has been stated that the Fi young produced by the cross of female albinos of race B with male cutleri were dark golden agouti in color, much darker than the cutleri parent. This darkening of the color per- sisted undiminished into the following generation (F2). Of the 58 colored Fi young derived from this cross none was as light in coloration as the cutleri grandparent. Hence it would appear that the darker coloration introduced by the cross, apparently through the albino parent, does not behave as a simple Mendelian character either domi- nant or recessive; otherwise pale-colored F2 young should have been produced. Whatever factors, Mendelian or otherwise, are responsible for the darkening of the pigmentation are evidently unconnected with the so-called color factor, since they are transmitted by albinos, which lack this factor. A very different result was observed in crosses of the same cutleri males with females of race C. The colored animals of race C are very pale cream-colored. The Ft young which they produced showed a more intense yellow than either parent, but were much lighter than the hybrids produced in the cross with race B albinos. (See plate 3.) Among their F2 young appeared some very light-colored individuals, 16 being recorded in a total of 56 young produced by pairs which pro- duced no albinos. The pale-colored young were not confined to any one colored class, but were recorded among the agoutis, blacks, cinna- mons, and yellows. (See table 11.) The proportion recorded is close to one-fourth, from which it would seem that dilution had been intro- duced as a recessive character by the cream guinea-pig grandparents. Since, however, C. cutleri is relatively pale in pigmentation, it is prob- able that some of the animals classified as pale were not "dilute," owing to a factor derived from the guinea-pig ancestor, but because of condi- tions derived from the cutleri ancestor. This statement applies to the young of matings which produced albinos as well as to those which did not. The significant thing is that more pale-pigmented young and those which excelled in paleness were obtained from those matings which did not involve albinsim. - IMIKHITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. .,. n DMtAmMm -..ty and dilution among the F2 young derived from tht cross 9 race CX d'cutleri. oung. I ( iiin.'tnion BUol How . Brow in jr*d jreltow la U Total in pain pro- ducing no albinos. Intense. 5 • > 5 2 1 40 Dilute. 8 3 8 2 1G From pairs pro- ducing albinos. Intense. 2S 6 13 10 6 7 70 Dilute. 4 1 4 2 11 It was expected thai albinos of race C would produce a much larger rtion of pale-colored grandchildren, but strange to say this expec- od was n- >i realised; 81 F2 colored young produced in matings which yielded albinoe ahowing that the guinea-pig characters had been eived through albino gametes) included only 11 pale-colored young, of these is recorded as being paler in color than the cutleri grandparent. It would appear, therefore, that the albino gametes of I mothers do not transmit the dilution seen in the cream-colored animal- of race ( . This would be a puzzling state of affairs had not Wright 1915) already discovered an easy explanation for it, viz, that dilution of the cream race is an allelomorph of albinism, and so can be transmitted in the same gamete with albinism. I mparing the I', hybrids derived from race C crosses with those ived from race B crosses, it is certain that the pigmentation of both darker than that of wild C. cutleri, but the intensity of the race B hybrid- much exceeds that of the race C hybrids. Among the race B hybrid- do evidence can be discovered of segregating Mendelian inten- among the race ( ' hybrids dilution segregates as a simple Mendelian recessive, precisely as dors albinism, but apparently no garnet.- transmits both dilution and albinism, for the reason that they alternative conditions of the same factor. Aside from the factorial difference in dilution, how does race B differ from race C? Apparently in no simple factorial way, but iii a genera] way as regards energy of pigment production, in which hybrids of both races surpass C. cutleri if differ quantitatively from each other. No Mendelian explanation • Si difference is at present justified by the observations made. 0) SIGN If 1 OF THE RESULTS OBSERVED. rmplete fertility of the hybrids produced by crossing wild Cavia i with the guinea-pig is in striking contrast with the sterility of bride between C. rvfi and the guinea-pig, as observed by Det- CROSSES OF CAVIA CUTLERI. 19 lefsen. This indicates that C. cutleri from Peru is the actual wild ancestor of the guinea-pig or closely related to that ancestor. Since, however, Nehring has reported that C. aperea (from Argentina) also produces fertile hybrids with the guinea-pig, it seems likely that these two species are closely related to each other and might interbreed freely if their respective ranges were not completely separated. It seems possible also that both species have contributed to the production of the domesticated form, or that still other species have shared in producing it. Further observations are needed to clear up this matter. It is evident that the mendelizing unit-character differences, which distinguish one variety of domesticated guinea-pig from another, also exist between guinea-pigs and the wild Cavia cutleri. They are inherited in precisely the same way among the hybrids produced by crossing guinea-pigs with C. cutleri as in crosses of one variety of guinea-pig with another — that is, they mendelize. It is evident that these variations have arisen by a process of retrogressive or loss variation. For example, in the matter of color varieties such as black, brown, yellow, and white, which (in relation to the parent form) are known to breed true without exception, it is evident that these have arisen by loss (or retrogressive modification) of physiological processes which occur in the wild species, since crosses with the wild form bring them back in a heterozygous state, after which they continue to form all possible permutations and recombinations with each other. Thus albinos of race C (which breed true inter se and without crossing with some other variety could pro- duce no other sort) if crossed with C. cutleri (which also breeds true) produce in F2 a definite series of color varieties. This series includes all the color varieties of guinea-pigs more commonly known, such as (1) golden agouti, (2) black, (3) cinnamon, (4) chocolate, (5) black-eyed yellow, (6) brown-eyed yellow, and (7) albino. The mode of origin of the color varieties of guinea-pigs (and by inference of other domesticated animals also) is therefore clear. These varieties have originated by loss variations or loss "mutations." Is this the means by which species themselves originate? Many biolo- gists have recently advocated this view, as, for example, Lotsy, Baur, and Bateson, but the present case affords rather strong evidence against it. The color varieties of guinea-pigs differ from Cavia rufescens and C. cutleri (undoubtedly distinct species) by the same mendelizing color-factors, but there is no evidence that these two species differ from each other by any color-factor. The two wild species are probably distinct enough to show interspecific sterility, since one is known to form sterile hybrids, the other fertile hybrids, in crosses with the guinea-pig. Their specific distinctness accordingly can not be due to such mendelizing factors as distinguish one domesticated variety from another, but to something more fundamental in character, though less striking in appearance. QfHUUl kN( B in GUINEA-PIGS. HYBRIDIZATION EXPERIMENTS WITH A RACE OF FERAL GUINEA PIGS FROM ICA, PERU. ORIGIN AND CHARACTERISTICS OF THE ICA RACE. Von Tschudi in 184 L, in bis Fauna of Peru, described, under the name B wild cavy found occurring in great numbers in the II, Bays thai the natives call it "cuy del monte," the . v. and regard it as the original of C.cobaye, the guinea-pig. Subeequenl writers carefully distinguish the C. cutleri of Von Tschudi from thai of Bennett, with which my wild cavies from Arequipa agree. the objects which I hoped to accomplish by the trip to Peru was to Irani more aboul the cavy which Von Tschudi reported as occur- ringal lea, and, if possible, to determine its relation to C.etriUri Bennett and to the guinea-pin. Through the kindly interesl of Messrs. W. R. Grace & Co. I was able I ire 3 wild-caughl cavies (a male and 2 females) from lea ; to bring them back with me to the Bussey Institution, where they • ■ produced a numerous progeny. animals were about the Bize of domesticated guinea-pigs, were very timid, and were -elf-colored golden agouti, in every respect similar in appearance to tame guinea-pigs of the color variety named. animals brought from lea produced 7 golden-agouti young, all similar to the parents in color, except that one bore a spot of red, firsl observed indication of contamination of the stock with char- found in domesticated guinea-pigs. That other indications re no1 observed in this first mating of the animals was probably due to the fact thai the male was homozygous for all other color factors, as subsequent matings of the females with a son of one of them by the original male proved thai both mother and son were heterozygous in that variation of the color factor which is seen in "red-eyed" guinea- pi- I istle, 191 1: Wright, 1915). The same matings with the son ible 12 proved that one of the two original females (9 503) tlso heterozygous in the agouti factor and transmitted white- otting, since she produced a black daughter which had one white foot. Three other inbred descendants of the original trio of lea animals have if white; two of them in addition bore spots of red, tricolors. One of the original trio of animals from lea when mated with '.">n:>, produced a son (c?575) which was tly rough-coated. Accordingly we have clear evidence that the stock derived from [ca was contaminated with at least 5 of the ily independent unit-factor variations which occur among doi d guinea-pigs and there can belittle doubt that it really has 1 wholly or in part from domesticated guinea-pig ancestors. GUINEA-PIGS FROM ICA. 21 Table 12. — Young produced by the three original lea guinea-pigs or their inbred descendants. A. Both parents golden agouti, one only heterozygous for red-eye (indicated by *). Father. 501 *505 *505 *505 *533 *533 Mother. *502 and *503 509 510 530 509 625 Total . Golden agouti young. 7 4 1 3 7 24 Black young. One young with spot of red. One young spotted with red and with white. B. Both parents golden agouti and heterozygous for red-eye. Father. 505 505 505 505 505 533 533 Mother. 502 503 504 507 605 529 540 Total . Golden agouti young. 10 9 1 8 1 5 4 38 Silver agouti young. 4 2 4 5 1 1 1 18 Black young. One slightly rough. One black with white foot. One spotted with red and with white. One spotted with white. C. Both parents silver agouti (red-eyed). Father. 565 565 565 565 565 565 Mother. 527 528 573 593 601 and 604 607 Silver agouti young. 5 8 1 2 3 1 Father. 569 602 79S 798 Mother. 587 and 588 60S 701 872 Total. Silver agouti young. 31 The question at once arises whether the stock obtained by me from lea was really a feral stock, in origin like the animals described by Von Tschudi, or whether they were present-day domesticated animals concerning whose origin I was deceived. Since I did not myself see the animals captured or see similar animals running at large and did not even visit lea, I can make no positive statement as to their feral origin, but I believe the report made to me by the agents of W. R. Grace & Co., that they were caught feral in the neighborhood of lea, to be correct for the following reasons: (1) The animals were placed INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. board our steamer during a stop made in the night at Pisco, the the abort line of railway which leads down from lea to the I found them the next morning in the "butcher-shop," con- signed from lea to W . EL ( brace & Co. in Callao. I conclude that they really did come from the ueighborhood of lea. (2) I saw no domesti- ted guinea-pigs in Peru which were self-colored like these animals. All domesticated ones which I saw in Peru, except albinos, were spotted with white, or with yellow, or both. Self varieties are not fancied in P< pu. \ arieties of this aort are not uncommon among the guinea-pigs kept by European and American fanciers, but apparently they have bushed only by careful and long-continued selection from the 1 Meek i iriginally introduced from South America. (3) The spotting ..ugh character which have cropped out as recessives among the descendants of the three lea animals are feebly expressed characters which appear to have been almost obliterated, but which still come to nor feebly under inbreeding. This is what we should expect to find in a feral race acted upon by natural selection, conspicuous nations like spotting tending to disappear. Tabu 13. — "an >. pure lea animals irhose malings are recorded in table 12. 1 ther. Individual. Father. Mother. Individual. Father. Mother. 9527 505 507 9 601 565 527 9- 9 528 505 503 c?602 565 527 9'-' 9ol".i 505 503 9 604 505 503 9504 601 r 503 9530 505 503 9 605 505 503 ■ produced 1 14 young, of which 62 have been golden agoutis, 49 sil- routis, and 3 blacks; 4 of the 114 have shown a small amount of white spotting, '1 have shown yellow spotting, and 1 has shown a ill amount of roughness of the coat. '1 he various matingn which have produced these young are classified in three group* in table 12, and the parentage of each animal which I pari in a mating i- shown in table 13, from which pedigrees may lily be drawn tracing back to the original trio. It will be observed m table L2 that silver agouti was derived from golden agouti as a and has bred true without exception (31 silver agouti young produced by silvr agouti parents). GUINEA-PIGS FROM ICA. 23 CROSSES BETWEEN THE ICA RACE AND GUINEA-PIGS OF RACE C. An albino male guinea-pig (cf 54) of race C was mated with 5 golden agouti females of the lea stock. It was hoped from this cross to learn as promptly as possible the gametic composition of the lea race, since race C contained a larger number of recessive Mendelian factors than any other race in the laboratory. In this hope we were not disap- pointed. Race C has already been described. It contains two different recessive variations of the color factor, dilution and albinism, which are allelomorphic with each other and with ordinary color, thus forming a system of triple allelomorphs, C, Cd, and Ca, with dominance in the order named (see Wright, 1915). It lacks agouti, black, and extension factors. Visibly the animals of this race are either brown-eyed cream or albino. Male 54 was an albino, bearing the color allelomorph Ca, which is recessive to the color allelomorph Cd found in brown-eyed cream individuals of race C. The mating between cf 54 and the 5 golden agouti females of the lea race produced 13 young, 7 of which were Table 14. -Fi result of mating the albino &5J+ of race C with golden agouti females of the lea race. Mother. Dark-eyed young. Golden agouti. Red-eyed young. Silver agouti. Sepia. 502 503 504 507 509 Total . . . 3 1 1 2 1 1 X 1 1 2 7 3 3 golden agouti (like the mothers), 3 silver agouti, and 3 a dull black or slate color, which will be called sepia. The silver agouti young were like those produced by lea animals bred inter se. The sepia young represented a new class not previously observed. In common with the silver agoutis they had no yellow in their fur. The ticking and spotting of silver agoutis was of white, as was also the spotting of the sepias, which had no ticking. It seemed probable, therefore, as proved to be the case, that the silver agouti and the sepia young differed from each other only in the presence or absence of the agouti factor. But these two classes of young taken together differ from golden agoutis in lacking yellow pigmentation with which the golden agouti fur is ticked. They also differ from golden agoutis in the intensity of the eye pigmentation, which is very great in golden agoutis and blacks, but ordinarily shows such reduction in silver agoutis and sepias that the eye by reflected light has a deep red glow. It will be convenient to distinguish them as red- j, IMIKKII W I IN i.l'INEA-PIGS. ,1 ,, being undent 1 thai the red eye is invariably associated with low in the ooat, I ,ur of the five [ca mothers which were mated with ^54 had pro- duced silver agouti red-eyed) young by lea mates. Each of them pril.j,. l-eyed young by o*54; 1 1 >ge1 her they produced 5 dark-eyed ■ . .l.l.-n agouti) and 6 red-eyed (silver agouti or sepia). The fifth fca mother 509) had produced 11 golden agouti young when mated with [ca males known to be heterozygous for silver agouti. able 12. This is good evidence that she did not carry red-eye as ., character and was accordingly homozygous for dark-eye. By '64 she produced 2 golden agouti young. From these several tacts it appears that dark-eyed lea animals capable of producing red-eyedyoung when mated inter se, produce equal Qumbers of dark-eyed and red-eyed young when mated to albinos, but produce qo albinos. This indicates that albinism is recessive both to red-eye and dark-eye, an indication which the F2 result confirms. It will be shown further that the three conditions are mutually allelomor- phic, bo thai a sygote may contain any two of the three, but not more. d-eye is in fad a fourth member of the albino series of allelomorphs, which include- the following conditions in order of dominance: (1) ordi- nary dark-eye and colored coat, such as is seen in Cavia cutleri and in golden agouti animals of the lea race; (2) dark-eye with dilute coat, d in colored animals of race C; (3) red-eye and non-yellow coat; l albino. (See Wright, 1915.) For convenience these allelomorphs may be designated by C, C<], Cr, and Ca. The cross of lea females with the albino o*54 involves animals of the formulae CC or CCr mated with an animal of the formula CaCa. The 7 golden agouti young are expected to be of the formula CCa; the 6 red-eyed young of the formula I I We may now compare the experimental with the expected results of breeding such animals in various ways. THE F, GENERATION. of the Fj silver agouti males (cf 517) was known from his pedi- • be heterozygous in four characters, viz, red-eye vs. albinism, non-agouti, black vs. brown, and extension vs. restriction. Hie formula was accordingly CrCaAaBbEe, and we should expect him form gametes of 16 different sorts, all equally numerous. This animal was mated with all three kinds of F! females, with the results shown in tabic 15. The golden agouti females produced 25 young, distributed among 10 classes very distinctly different in appearance. -outi females were known from pedigree to be hetero- for the Bame 4 factors as c?517, but to contain a different allelo- morph for albinism. Both he and they carried albinism as a recessive • but whereas he carried red-eye (Cr) as its dominant allelo- rph, they carried dark-eye it he ordinary condition of the color GUINEA-PIGS FROM ICA. 25 factor, viz, C). His gametes accordingly could transmit either Cr or Ca, but theirs would transmit either C or Ca. Accordingly their young should be in the ratio 2 dark-eyed to 1 red-eyed to 1 albino. The observed numbers were 13:8:4. Each of these three groups might theoretically contain 8 different kinds of individuals, but certain of these would be visibly indistinguishable. The classes visibly different which might themselves be expected to be composite are black-eyed reds and brown- Table 15. — Ft surprising- outi p females, cf517 had 9 young of 4 different color the maximum Dumber of classes expected being 6. «pia T females, cf517 had 35 young, distributed among 6 dif- ,Vnnt color classes, as expected. Summarizing the results from all three kinds of mating*, we 6nd that the F2 young of c?ol7 number 69, distributed among 1 1 of the 12 expected classes of young, the missing rh- l.ri.m one in which the expectation is for 0.6 of an individual, lv more than an even chance for the production of such an indi- vidual in the Dumber of young recorded. (See table 15.) I >A , , , ] ft— Young product d by red-eyed white parents mated inter se. Red-eyed Albino Father. Mother. white young. young. 567 571 2 1 .VT<> 726 10 0 77 1 747 5 2 771 758 3 3 771 775 8 0 B42 571 2 0 B49 T 851 otal 2 0 32 6 A word as to the number of classes expected may not be out of place. The dark-eyed classes expected are 6, identical with those expected from the cross of race C animals with wild Cavia cutleri. (Compare p. I I The nui nber of classes expected among the red-eyed young is 1 leas, namely 5, because red-eyed whites which have brown pigment in the an not be distinguished (except by breeding-test or post rom those which have black pigment in the eye, the quantity • present being too small, and the coat in both cases white. the whole the agreement between expected and observed in this i n nt is so good as to preclude the idea that any coupling or associatioD occurs among the 4 unit factors involved in the cross. This experiment produced 4 color varieties of guinea-pig previously unknown to me, viz. the 4 red-eyed classes other than silver agoutis, which had already been obtained from the uncrossed lea race. (See plat.- 1. _\ and 5. The eve has a similar appearance in all the red- Bhowing a deep-red glow by reflected light. The silver ■iiti variety, as already explained, differs from golden agouti in the if the fur. which is white in silver agouti, instead of red or Aden agouti. Sepia as compared with black has a more fad.d appearance, approaching chocolate on the sides of the body and belly, but always darker and unmistakably black above. Silver cinna- GUINEA-PIGS FROM ICA. 27 mon (or "red-eyed cinnamon," plate 5, fig. 31) differs from silver agouti in having brown hairs ticked with white instead of black hairs ticked with white. It is one of the handsomest of guinea-pig varieties. Red- eyed chocolate is indistinguishable from dark-eyed chocolate, except in eye color. The red-eyed whites all look alike, though they may differ considerably in factorial composition. Their production in this experi- ment was a complete surprise to us and very puzzling until the sugges- tion was made (I think by Mr. Wright) that an essential feature of the red-eyed variation was the absence of yellow color from the fur. It was then realized that a " yellow" animal with red eyes and "non- yellow" fur must of necessity have white fur. This suggestion was immediately put to the test by mating the red-eyed white d"576 with 3 dark-eyed cream females. They produced 12 young, of which 5 were brown-eyed cream, 2 black-eyed cream, 3 red-eyed white, and 2 albino. No young were produced which had coats of any other color than yellow! Hence it is clear that red-eyed whites do not transmit the extension factor.1 Table 17. — Results of mating red-eyed white individuals with albinos. Father, red- eyed. Mother, albino. Red-eyed young. Albino young. Silver agouti. Sepia. Silver cinnamon. Choco- late. White. 567 567 567 567 567 576 576 576 564 568 572 177 711 1430 1439 1446 1 2 1 1 1 4 1 4 2 2 7 1 1 2 T otal 3 3 1 2 5 19 This same red-eyed white c?576 was also mated with 3 albino females of race B, which carry the extension factor. Both parents, it will be observed, were white, one having red eyes, the other pink eyes. This mating produced 7 young, of which 3 were red-eyed with silver- agouti-colored coats and 4 were albinos. The production of colored young in this case shows that red-eyed white animals may transmit all that is necessary for the production of a colored coat except the exten- sion factor, which the albino parents supplied. The red-eyed white d"576 was evidently heterozygous for the black factor, since, when he was mated with brown-eyed cream females, he produced both black-eyed and brown-eyed cream young. Another xAs a further test of red-eyed whites, two other red-eyed white males (615 and 616) were mated with several different red or yellow coated females. They produced 9 red or yellow young, 5 red- eyed young, and 5 albino young, a result completely in accord with that given by c? 576. 2S INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. red-eyed white male, 507, an F3 descendant of the albino d"54, race C, was found to be homozygous for brown (table 17). What pigment his eyes contained was undoubtedly brown, for w7hen he was mated with 3 albino females descended from the albino cf 54, race C, he produced 1 silver cinnamon and 2 red-eyed chocolate young, besides 5 red-eyed white and 8 albino young. The entire absence of black-colored young indicates that this male, as well as his albino mates, transmitted the capacity to form brown but not black pigmentation. When, however, this same male (567) was mated with an albino derived from race B, which never produces brown individuals, there were obtained 3 sepia- colored young with red eyes, besides 7 albinos, showing that when the mother transmitted black, this male produced black-pigmented young, black being dominant over brown which he himself transmitted. Table IS. — Results of mating a red-eyed white male xoith brown-eyed cream females. Father. Mother. Young. Black-eyed cream. Brown-eyed cream. Red-eyed white. Albino. 576 576 576 842 -46 M250 762 870 1 1 2 1 2 1 1 2 2 1 Tot al 2 6 3 3 Both the males wrhose matings have just been described, viz, 567 and 576, were heterozygous in albinism, since when mated wdth albinos they produced about 50 per cent of albino young. They were evidently of the formula CrCa. If red-eyed white animals of this formula should be mated with each other we should expect individuals to be produced which are homozygous for red-eye, i. e., are of formula CrCr. Twro probably homozygous red-eyed females of this sort have been discov- ered in mating red-eyed white animals inter se. One of them (9 726, table 16) produced 10 young, all red-eyed white, in matings with c?576, known to be heterozygous for albinism. Had this female formed albino gametes she should have produced 25 per cent of albino young in the matings mentioned. It seems probable, therefore, that she did not form Mich gametes. The F3 9 775 (table 16) was probably like- wise homozygous, since her mate is known to have been heterozygous for albinism, but >he produced no albinos in a total of 8 young. In the foregoing account nothing has been said concerning spotting with white or with yellow; nevertheless spotting of both sorts occurred among certain of the F, and F2 young obtained from the lea crosses. Since the uncrossed lea race contained spotted animals of both sorts, it k n..t surprising that the cross-bred descendants of this race should GUINEA-PIGS FROM ICA. 29 do the same. Race C, like the lea race, contains only an occasional individual sparingly spotted with white; yellow spotting is of course not visible in a race like C, which contains only yellow or albino indi- viduals. It will suffice to say that the cross-breds, like the parent races, consisted principally of self-colored individuals, and that only an occa- sional dark-eyed individual bore white markings, which in no case were extensive, but were usually limited to a white foot. Among the red- eyed individuals, white spotting was commoner and more extensive, which might seem surprising, unless one remembers that in red-eyed individuals it is impossible to distinguish true white spotting from yellow spotting, since both produce uncolored areas in the coat. Com- plications of this nature make this cross unfavorable for the study of the inheritance of spotting. SUMMARY ON THE ICA RACE. 1. The "lea race" of guinea-pigs consists of descendants of 1 male and 2 female golden agoutis obtained from the vicinity of lea, Peru, in 1911, and reported to have been caught wild. These animals are supposed to have been descendants of guinea-pigs long since escaped from domestication. 2. This explanation is supported by the observation that within the lea race have cropped out 5 Mendelian variations which are common among domesticated guinea-pigs, viz, (l)the "red-eye" variation, oneof the four allelomorphic forms of the color factor in guinea-pigs; (2) the "non-agouti" allelomorph of the agouti factor; (3) the factor which produces rough coat; (4) the factor for white spotting; and (5) the factor for yellow spotting. 3. An albino guinea-pig of race C differing from wild guinea-pigs by 4 recessive Mendelian characters was crossed with golden agouti females of the lea race. From this cross were obtained in F2 all except one of the expected recombinations of the 4 unit-factor differences between the races crossed. Leaving out of consideration spotting with white and with red, which occurred among some of the hybrids as well as in the uncrossed lea race, there occurred 5 easily distinguishable classes of dark-eyed young and 5 classes of red-eyed young, besides albinos. Only one "expected" class of F2 young was missing, the occurrence of which among other races is well known. There is almost an even chance for its failure to appear in this experiment in the number of young recorded. 4. The four color factors involved in the cross and their allelomorphs are: A, a = agouti, non-agouti; B, b = black, brown; C, Cr = full color, red-eye; E, e = extension (of black or brown), restriction. ;;il IMIKHITANCK IX GUINEA-PIGS. Th) e capable theoretically of forming 16 diflferent combinations, ollows, heterozygous combinations being omitted. The appearance ontaining each of these several combinations is indicated ()pj, pective combinations. AlK'K. golden agouti = wild type. Single mutations. \!U •,.. blade-eyed red. AbCF:, cinnamon. silver agouti. aBCE, black. Double mutations. AIH'rr. red-eyed white. AbCrE, silver cinnamon. !'.. chocolate. AbCe, brown-eyed red. aBCrE, red-eyed sepia. aBCe, black-eyed red Triple mutations. AbCVe, redn yed white, red-eyed white. abCe, brown-eyed red. abCrE, red-eyed chocolate. Quadruple mutation. abCre, red-eyed white. • he I6different combinations, 2 produce black-eyedred individuals indistinguishable except by breeding test; the same is true regarding 1 .r< iwn-eyed reds. Four other combinations identical with these, except for the substitution of Cr for C, produce red-eyed whites, which visibly all alike but which breed differently. Three of the four kinds of red-eyed whites have been identified by breeding test; no doubt the fourth can easily be obtained. The fact that the several classes of red-eyed whites look alike, and that the two kinds of black-eyed reds look alike, and further, that the two kinds of brown-eyed reds look alike, reduces the number of visibly distinguishable classes from 16 to 1 1 , all excepl one of which have been recorded from this single experi- ment. The experiment also produced albinos which theoretically Bhould 1" different formula), if in the formula Ca is everywhere substituted for its allelomorphs C or Cr. No attempt has been made to distinguish the several expected classes of albinos by breeding tests, the only certain means of identifying them. Hie cl menl observed between theoretical and recorded cumbers of V ■ offspring in this cross lends no support to the idea that ciation or linkage occurs among the 4 factorial variations involv GUINEA-PIGS FROM AREQUIPA. 31 HYBRIDIZATION EXPERIMENTS WITH A DOMESTICATED GUINEA-PIG FROM AREQUIPA. While in Arequipa, in December 1911, I purchased in the cabin of a native living near the observatory a pair of domesticated guinea-pigs about one-third grown and perhaps 2 or 3 months old. These animals resembled the ordinary pied guinea-pigs kept for pets or laboratory use in Europe and North America. The female was a tricolor, red, white, and black, and was rough-coated of grade B (Castle, 1905, p. 57). The male was a dilute-pigmented, agouti-marked tricolor (yellow agouti,1 cream, and white), and smooth-coated. This pair of animals was suc- cessfully transported to the Bussey Institution, where they produced 3 litters, of 1, 3, and 2 young respectively. The young of the first 2 litters died at birth; the third litter consisted of 2 males, and as the mother died soon afterward it was impossible to propagate the family farther for lack of females. Of the 6 young produced, 3 were rough-coated and 3 smooth, showing the mother to have been hetero- zygous for rough coat, a dominant character (Castle, 1905). Three were golden agouti and white and three tricolor, one being golden agouti red and white, the other two silver agouti yellow and white. MALE 1002 AND HIS Fx OFFSPRING. The father of this family of guinea-pigs (of 1002) proved to be an animal of great vigor and vitality. Although born in Peru (about September 1911) and brought to North America in mid-winter, he has successfully escaped the ravages of disease among our guinea-pigs throughout the rigors of four New England winters and is still vigorous and active. In crosses with other races of guinea-pigs he has sired several hundred young and is now being mated with females which are simultaneously his daughters, his granddaughters, and his great- granddaughters! By repeated back-crosses such as these a race has been established which derives its inherited characters largely from this one animal. This race will be designated the "Arequipa" race. Crosses of 0*1002 and repeated back-crosses with his female descen- dants have permitted a very full analysis of the factorial constitution of this animal. He possesses either as dominant or as recessive char- acters a majority of the Mendelian variations of guinea-pigs, including one not previously known to occur in any animal other than mice, viz, xIt should he noted that "silver agoutis" may he of two different sorts: (1) dark-eyed silver agouti with cream-colored hair-tips, and (2) red-eyed silver agouti with white hair-tips. The two varieties resemble each other somewhat and it often requires close observation to discriminate between them, but genetically they are quite distinct. Only the former sort was known to me previous to the Peruvian expedition, and the term "silver agouti" as used in my 1905 paper and by fanciers generally refers to this. It would be better, I think, to use the term cream agouti or yellow agouti for such agouti animals as develop pale yellow in the fur and to restrict the term silver agouti to those which are non-yellow. INHERITANCE IN (iUINEA-PIGS. the pink-eye variation with colored coat, first brought to the attention cientiste in the case of mice through the experiments of Darbishire 1902 . \ similar variation has, however, since been found to occur in rate | astle,1914). Thenumberof factors in which ark-eyed sepia X silver agouti. . Silver agouti X silver agouti Silver agouti X sepia (red-eyed) . . Total 2 10 2 12 28 7 18 8 25 16 53 28 Dark-eye X red-eve 6 4 22 15 18 35 9 19 Red-eye X red-eye Nature of Fi mating. Pink-eyed. Red-and-pink- eyed. Albino. Agouti Non- agouti. Agouti. Non- agouti. < ■< .hlen agouti X black 1 1 3 2 2 4 11 7 11 Golden agouti X silver agouti . . . . clc X silver agouti Yellow agouti X silver agouti . . rk-eyed sepia X silver agouti . . Silv r agouti X silver agouti gouti X Bepia (red-eyed) . . •al 1 1 5 2 33 Ic-eye x red-eye 1 1 c 15 • ■ r. d-eye £ XO we summarize the matings in which every mother and father is w.i to luivo been capable of producing albinos, we have 96 colored albino young; expected 93 to 31— a very good agreement with bation. iimmariiring the matings between a red-eyed male and a dark-eyed male known to have been capable of producing red-eyed young, we gel Id dark-eyed and 27 red-eyed; expected 33.5 and 33.5. This GUINEA-PIGS FROM AREQUIPA. 35 apparent deficiency of red-eyed young may have been due to our failure at first to distinguish dark-eyed sepias from red-eyed sepias, which look very much alike when first born. In the summary all sepias are treated as dark-eyed unless a specific record in the ledger indicates that they were red-eyed. Matings yielding pink-eyed young produced 17 non-pink-eyed and 6 pink-eyed young, which is good agreement with the expected 3 to 1 ratio. BACK-CROSS AND OTHER OFFSPRING OF ^1002. Male 1002 was mated with certain of his Fi daughters, producing 90 young of 10 different color classes, as indicated in table 21. He was later mated with certain of the female young produced by the matings last described, these females being both his daughters and his grand- daughters and so "f-blood" Arequipa tracing back to himself. (See Table 21. — Classification of young of (3*1002 by his Fi daughters (table 19). Mothers. Intense dark- eyed. Dilute dark- eyed. Red-eyed. Pink-eyed. Red-and- pink-eyed. Golden agouti. Black. Yellow agouti. Sepia. Silver agouti. Sepia. Agouti. Non- agouti. Agouti. Non- agouti. 4 black 1 6 3 2 13 8 3 1 3 7 11 5 3 1 7 2 2 3 2 1 2 1 2 1 1 sepia (dark-eyed). 6 silver agouti 3 sepia (red-eyed) . . Total 1 6 26 14 16 13 7 3 3 1 table 22.) The table 22 matings have produced to date (April 1915) 61 young, distributed in 9 of the 10 classes represented among the table 21 young. The classes of young recorded in tables 21 and 22 are the same as those represented among the F2 young (table 20), with the exception of albinos, which are never produced by & 1002, since he does not transmit albinism, which is recessive to both of its allelomorphs. As regards the characters in which 6*1002 is heterozygous, there is evidence from tables 20 to 22 that in the case of each he forms equal numbers of gametes bearing the dominant and the recessive allelo- morphs respectively. By agouti daughters he has had 50 agouti and 15 non-agouti young; expected, 49 and 16. By non-agouti daughters he has had 45 agouti and 41 non-agouti young; expected, 43 of each. Combining these totals with those recorded in table 19, we find that in all matings with non-agouti animals he has sired 101 agouti and 73 non-agouti young, a not improbable chance deviation from the ex- pected equality of the two classes. By red-eyed daughters cf 1002 has had 51 dark-eyed and 40 red-eyed young. Adding to this result that recorded in table 19 (last category INHER1 I \\< i [N '.I [NEA-PIGS. of matingB), which has the Bame expectation of rod-eyed young (50 per t b total of 68 dark-eyed and 51 red-eyed, fairly good agreement with the expected equality. Bj dark-eyed daughters which have produced red-eyed young and bown thai they transmil eit I hm- red-eye or albinism, •_> 1 4 3 2 1 1 1 1 1 1 2 1 2 3 1 2 1 1 1 1 3 2 1 3 1 2 1 1 uti 2 nilvrt Kgouti 2 * 2 DOn-SCOUtJ (pink- 2 red-and- pinl tal 4 1 16 6 10 8 7 4 5 0 By pink-eyed daughters, d" 1002 has produced 7 pink-eyed young and B with eyes not pink — complete agreement with the expected equality. By daughters not pink-eyed, but which nevertheless are clearly hetero- lygoue in pink-eye, he has produced 23 pink-eyed and 58 not-pink-eyed young; expected, 20 and 61 an excess of pinks capable of explanation on the Bame ground as the excess of red-eyed young. dilute-colored daughters o" 1002 has produced 52 dilute-colored young, bul do intense-colored one-, as expected, since dilution is ive to intensity. By intense-colored daughters heterozygous for dilution he has produced in intense and 9 dilute young, equality being ad. MISCELLANEOUS MATINGS OF THE DESCENDANTS OFo*1002. MatingB of the descendants of 71002 beyond the F2 generation were made chiefly with a view- to test further the genetic character of the new Their results are presented in tables 23 to 28 and serve to firm the interpretations already offered. GUINEA-PIGS FROM AREQUIPA. 37 Matings of red-eyed animals inter se have in most cases produced only red-eyed or albino young, but two matings have also produced pink- and-red-eyed young, i. e., animals which are pink-eyed but develop no yellow in their fur, in which last respect they differ from ordinary pink-eyed and agree with ordinary red-eyed. (See tables 24 and 27.) Table 23. — Young produced by matings of red-eyed males, descended from c?1002, with dark-eyed females of race B. Nature of mating. Dark-eyed. Red-eyed. Albino. Golden agouti. Black. Silver agouti. Sepia. Silver agouti X black (homozygous) .... Silver agouti X black (heteiozygous in albinism) 1 20 5 7 6 9 7 10 Sepia (red-eyed) X black (homozygous) . . Total 21 18 9 7 10 Most of the red-eyed animals, when bred inter se, produce albino as well as red-eyed young, showing themselves to be heterozygous for albinism and so of the formula CrCa. This is not surprising when we recall that all the Fx red-eyed animals must by hypothesis be of this formula, and that two-thirds of the F2 red-eyed should be of the same sort. In a few matings of red-eyed with red-eyed, which failed to produce albino young (table 24), it is probable that one or both parents Table 24. — Young produced by matings inter se of red-eyed descendants ofc?1002. (See also table 27). Nature of mating. Red-eyed young. Albino young. Silver agouti. Sepia. 56 18 19 6 9 22 11 Total 74 34 33 were homozygous for red-eye. Matings of red-eyed with albino animals (table 25), which failed to produce albinos in 6 or more young, afford clear criteria for red-eyed animals free from albinism and so of formula CrCr. Only one mating of a red-eyed animal with an albino has pro- duced pink-eyed young. (See table 27.) The red-eyed parent in this case (c?48) was mated with 4 other albinos (all of race B) without producing pink-eyed young, but only red-eyed (13) and albinos (17). The female which produced pink-eyed young was his sister, derived like INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. himself from paicnti known to transmil pink-eye This indicates that the character pink-eye in guinea-pigB (as in mice) may be transmitted |,v ultiinos. The fart should be emphasized that the pink-eyed young Tab1 ) . , ... lueedbyred^yed descendants of &1008 matedivith albinos. [See also tabl 97.) Nature of red-eyed parent. Red-eyed young. Silver agouti. BQver agouti, hefc as fox albinism 21 . heterosygoufl for albinism Silver agouti (homoiygouB for red-eye) ,ua for red-eye) Total. is Sepia, 11 31 11 20 39 73 Albino young. 17 34 51 produced m this mating were also red-eyed, i. e., were non-yellow, for red-eyed animal- may carry pink-eye as a recessive character, and con- rsely pink-eyed may carry red-eye as a recessive character. How- ,-. if these recessive characters crop out as recessive individuals from a mating of two like parents with each other, it can in either case :r only in the form of the double recessive, both pink- and red-eyed. Xai -• Young produced by pink-eyed descendants of t about a year and a half all cutleri individuals in the laboratory, whether <>f pun- race or hybrids, were weighed two or three times a month. In this way records were obtained from which growth , ,:, gee of weight, etc., can be deduced. The repeated and -went weighings allow the detection of periods of depression due to illness or poor feeding. Due allowance has been made for all such observations, as well as for increase in weight of females through pregnancy. Nevertheless, observations on weight are at best not i dRaceB H 9 Race B. <-• 6 Cutleri «•«.' 9 Cutleri .Day$ 40 SO 120 160 200 2-10 2S0 320 360 400 I 1 rowth-curves of C- cutleri and of race B guinea-pigs, the growth-curve of each sex being shown separately. altogether satisfactory, since they are subject to fluctuation through conditions of food, accumulations of fat when maturity has been reached, etc. Greater value attaches to the bone measurements of fully adult individuals (over 1 year old) so far as individual varia- bility i> concerned. But the observations on weight afford a basis entirely satisfactory for the determination of average sizes and average growth curve- in different classes of hybrids. Incidentally they afford ontrol on the bone measurements, for they indicate cases of abnormal through disease, fighting, or other cause) and allow of either remedying condition- or rejecting suspicious material. Pure ctUU n young of both sexes are of about the same average weight birth, vis, :>() grams (see fig. 1). The females at first grow a little than the male-, a fact perhaps correlated with their earlier sexual maturity. At about 50 days of age the two sexes are of practically the -lit. the male- having again caught up with the females, and SIZE. 45 subsequently the males are heavier. The average adult weight of a female is about 400 grams, that of a male about 420 grams. Race B animals of both sexes weigh on the average about 80 grams at birth (see fig. 1), but females grow at first a little faster than males, so that between 10 and 50 days of age females are slightly heavier. But the males soon catch up with the females and from 50 days on are heavier. The same difference between the growth curves of the two sexes is observable here, as in Cavia cutleri. The phenomenon is pos- sibly a general one among mammals. Earlier maturity of the female is attended by more rapid growth, but the ultimate weight attained by males is greater. There is no indication in our observations that the attainment of sexual maturity is followed by any slowing-up of the growth rate in either sex. In the growth of both C. cutleri and of race B, as in other growth- curves to be described, the curve is at first concave upward, but later becomes convex upward. This agrees with observations on rabbits, fowls, and other organisms, and its significance has been discussed elsewhere (Castle et at., 1909). 1 Weight in rf Fr Cut x Band C Oram 800 s 6 Race B. dF^Ci it.xBandC. 600 S Cutleri. 400 200 Age in Days 40 80 120 160 200 240 280 320 360 400 Fig. 2 — Growth curves of race B and cutleri males and of their male hybrids, both Fi and F2. Fj hybrid males (from the cross and 3 1 and] >late6.) In every case F{ exceeds the means of both parent races, but I' approximates the intermediate between them, which n exceeds by a fraction of a millimeter only. In no case is the I mean as great as the race B mean— that of the larger parent race. These facts, like the weight curves, indicate (1) that, so far as heredity oncerned, an exact intermediate between the parent races would SIZE. 49 Si • Hi -e s a a £ s 6b a a. -2 S S OS a dig s =0 a C5 « •noij -BiAap pjBpuB;g 13.50 19.75 16.85 17.20 9.45 15.05 12.15 20.00 12.10 20.65 15.15 17.75 10.00 10.00 11.20 22.65 23.55 24.55 •UBaj^ 51.55 58.14 57.70 54.35 52.91 60.35 61.20 57.26 61.92 60.44 64.40 61.86 57.45 60.20 58.84 58.10 62.20 62.17 ■JKJOJ, O X •* CO t^ CO CO -* GO i-h t^ CO X t> f O X t-- ^h CI CI CO tONN rH) T* N U5 i-H S J_, "o a 03 a a 03 L. U~ a 03 s | 1 73 a> pa CD S3 o * ' Z9-0 ' Z9 (N 6 99-5 ' 99 ^H f-H f 99-0 ' 99 6 ' 59-5 ' 59 TJH —4 ^'59-059 Tj< rt ■ i-H 6 ' ^9-5 ' f9 i-H CO • • CO ff9-Of9 * ■ r H rH IO CI ■ rH CO 6 ' S9-5 ' £9 H rH Mh CO -^ ■ i-H fr"£9~0'89 ■ c 1HH CI CO CI io ■ i-H 6 ' S9-5 ' S9 H I— 1 CI -^ CO m f S9-0 ' S9 • T r ci ■ CI • IO • i— 1 6' 19-5 19 1 - CI CO "O r-l Ol • • CI ^19-0 19 ' • T f m Tf CO -H CO • CI I— 1 1— 1 t— ( 6 09-5 09 ■ T c • ■ t~ - l- CO rH • CO • CI i-H i-H i-H CO fr' 09-0 '09 • i- i i— 1 • c 6 CI ■ CI • in • • CI i-H 6 65-5 '65 • c 3 CI ■ c 11 CO rt rj< ■ t^ • CI CI CO ^H f 65-0 65 • ^ i 'I1 • t- - i-h i— i Tf •CI 1— 1 • »-H I-H 6 ' 85-5 ' s'5 • c 3 CI • 0! 3 ■ rf CO • CO ^'85-0'85 • c 3 •* • c 3 rH t*< ■* i-H r- 1 rH 6 Z5-5 Z5 < 0O ■ CO CI CI CI «-H f ' Z5-0 ' Z5 • c 1 H rH • C 1 • ^h H ;- *'se-o-ze I-H i-H 6 18 -'.■ IS o» ■ i-H CO fr 18-0 [8 i-H ■ ^H 6 08-9 08 CM • <— ( CN roe oos (M 6 ►<> * 1 M : P i jc . i . i :p ■ < ■ < ■ • . « i> h - - 2 C qp u c J C 9 - H 1 K ) J -* a £& j 3 p 3 < 1 1 - ) < j i 3 J • M H r D C : P 3 J < 0 - r g H n't X c h e IS J a -• < ) y V -1 CI n t > » qp IS - - f - q D J < r IS D C 'I. ' t ; < :p • < • < • < . i : > • i • < it » c 3c H ( 5 ( 3P IS C i < i ■ - ■■ <> a < j < - - hP - C ) H 3 q D < H - < H »- >r ^ . :p • • • * > ■ * u 5c - t a ; qp; IS 0 J C 3 c H i < > a c - - £ps ) 3 1 > i < s 3 H ~» 3 SIZE. 51 to Si 1 1 a no "5 I S CO •uori 12.05 12.50 10.35 15.60 8.20 10.70 9.80 14.20 13.35 15.30 10.05 11.80 10.75 9.05 10.85 16.10 17.60 17.80 •m38j\[ 38.45 41.16 42.63 40.38 38.77 42.39 43.57 41.32 44.07 42.95 45.16 43.15 42.64 43.49 42.06 42.90 44.63 43.44 •pn<>x CM © CO CO t- CM Ci ■* COi-H iO •<# GOt^-* Ot^t-^ HNNM iO —l CM CM tjh NlO ,--, S iH 3 o a s 3 O* 5 (H V-i a •— . in V J 'a _g 0) CO 55 e8 5 t'ZH'ZJ- »— 1 • i-H 6'9H'W CM i-H i—l • i-H ^'9^-0'9^ ■ tH -r-l CM i—l 6'fiHfi^ CM CM 00 i— t 1—1 rS^-O^ 1—1 i— 1 T— i io i— i • f— t r-l ■ CM 6^-9'^ ■ rH HH Tf CO • IO r 1-1 tH CO * ff-0'ff • f— I • CO CO "# »0 IO CO i-H T-l 68fr-9'81? i— t CO i— 1 • CO IO i-H CO r-l O • i—l i — 1 CMi-H i—l ^"8^-0'8^ l-H T}H tPCM'-i CO iO CM t~» cOi-i- CMi-HCM 6'2^-9^ l-H CM CO CM "# CM CO Tt< i— 1 00 CM CO CO CM ^2^-0^ >o ■* O ■ ■ CO CO CI l-H ■* 61^-91^ IO CM CO Ci CD CO ■>* T*l NH ■ nH'i^ co cm co CO ■* >o 1— 1 1— 1 t-H i— l 6'OH'O^ CM (N CM NHUJ 1— 1 T-H T— 1 i— I ^Of-0'0* CO CO ■-! CM CM 1— 1 t-H 6 68-9 68 CO CM CO • i-H i— l i— 1 i—l ^•68-068 CO • iO CM CM 688-9'88 i-H CO rt CM f-88-088 i— 1 CM 6'Z8-9ZS - CM ^Z8-0Z8 i—l 6"98-9'98 i— 1 f 98-0 ' 98 i— < 6 98-9 '98 i-f co c 03 3 3 o o pq o O C c m 01 c (4 X 13 o 6 u o pq o o 03 tn X o fa O a *^ 3 o pq CO o t- Q 0 pq co o 33 J- X = u fa" d c pq CO S3 X c pq c V X 6 CO H < fa o PQ CO a a u X D nrk group. (]) Black. ty black =dark neutral gray. blue ■ neutral gray. White. COLOR. 63 b. Dark group — Continued. (2) Black. Sepia3 = 16"'w, warmer and darker than clove brown, 17"' m. Sepia6 = 16"'Z, warmer and lighter than clove brown, 17'" m. Sepia9 = 17"'% hair brown, slightly purer, however. Sepial2 = 17""6, light drab, somewhat purer. Sepia15 = 17""/, pale drab gray, somewhat purer. White. (3) Brown =15" m, bister, 15"m, but somewhat warmer and duller. Brown3 = 15"% between army brown, 13"% and buffy brown, \T"i. Brown6 = 17"'fe, somewhat duller than avellaneous, 17'"6. Brown9 = 17""/? White. DEFINITIONS OF EYE COLORS. (1) Black: black iris and pupil. Dark red : black iris, dark-red pupil in favorable lights. Red : partially transparent iris, red pupil in most lights. Pink : transparent iris, pink reflection through both iris and pupil. (2) Brown: brown iris, dark-red pupil. Brown-red: partially transparent brown iris, red pupil. Pink: as above. HEREDITY OF FUR AND EYE COLOR. COLOR FACTORS OF GUINEA-PIGS. Considerable work has been done on the inheritance of color varia- tions in guinea-pigs. The numerous colors which have been listed and several patterns in which these colors may be arranged have been found to be due in the main to relatively few hereditary factors. Some of these factors determine effects which are very easily defined. Thus, any guinea-pig which is homozygous for factor Ca is an albino with pink eyes and white fur, regardless of the presence of any combination of other known factors. On the other hand, certain factors determine nothing except in combination with other factors. Factor E may be present in guinea-pigs of any known color variety whatever. It can only be said that its presence is a necessary condition for the develop- ment of more than a trace of dark pigmentation in the fur. The color which results from a given combination of factors can be made clear most easily by classifying the factors into a series of groups. The following classification is based upon the factors in the rodents which have been most studied, viz, guinea-pigs, mice, rats, and rabbits. CLASSIFICATION OF COLOR FACTORS. 1. Factors which affect the distribution and intensity of color largely irre- spective of the kind of color. A. Factors which govern the distribution of color as opposed to no color (white) in patterns in the fur, in individual hairs, and in the eyes. B. Factors which govern the intensity of general color development within colored areas of fur and eyes. 2. Factors which govern the differentiation between yellow and dark colors in colored areas of the fur. 3. Factors which determine the kind of dark color in the areas with dark pigmentation in fur and eyes, without influence on yellow areas. ,,| INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. COLOR VS. WHITE (I A). Probably dilution of the type of the blue and dilute yellow mice and rabbits' and maltose cats belongs here, rather than in IB, since the effect Beams to be due to the distribution of pigment within the indi- vidual hairs rather than to any effect on the actual pigment granules. Most of the factors which belong in this class, however, are those which determine patterns of white as opposed to areas which are colored under most combinations of other factors. In this class are such fac- tors as mi the one hand determine a self-colored coat, and on the other black-eyed whites, as in mice; white patterns, as in hooded rats, Dutch and English rabbits; or scattered white hairs, as in silvered guinea- pigs. In cases where several independently inherited white patterns have arisen it is evident that there can be no single factor which alone determines self. The "self" allelomorphs of the white-pattern factors can merely be defined as conditions for self. Where more than one white-pattern factor is present in an animal, combination patterns are produced. ( lcar-cnt Mendelian factors which belong to this group are known in mice, rats, and rabbits, but none have been isolated in guinea-pigs, although irregular blotching and silvering with white are common. The >vml >< .1 Z will be used to represent an assemblage of unanalyzed factors. 1 . an assemblage of unanalyzed factors which determine white spotting. INTENSITY OF GENERAL COLOR DEVELOPMENT (IB). In this group fall albinism and its variations. These factors affect all color, but not wholly irrespective of the kind of color. There are several peculiarities which are discussed more fully in a later section (page 70). The most important is the fact that the level of intensity of the color factor at which yellow can develop at all is higher than the threshold for black or brown. This does not affect the differentiation of the fur into yellow and dark pigmentation areas by factors of group 2, but involves the result that with certain albino series factors, yellow areas appear white, while dark areas are quite strongly colored. Indeed, in albinism itself, dark pigmentation areas can often be distinguished from yellow areas by a slight sootiness in the former, absent in the latter. ' Detenninei the highest intensity of color of skin, fur, and eye which is to be found with a given array of other factors; dominant over Cd, Cr, and Ca, where distinguishable in its effects. In the following table, and in the similar tables under Cd,Cr, and Ca, given the ranges of intensity in the yellow, black, and brown series to which these colors develop when the factor under consideration is present. In the case of black and brown, factor P is assumed to be present. When p is present, black and brown undergo a two-fold dilution. P is also considered present in the case of i j • ■•< "](,r. "» ellnw series red,, to yellows in guinea-pigs; yeUow3 to cream6 in Cavia riilleri. Black series blacks to blacks- Brown series browns to browns- 1 •<• color — black, brown. COLOR. 65 Cd. Determines an intensity of yellow distinctly lower than does C, an intensity of dark pig- mentation usually, but not always lower than does C, and an intensity of eye color rarely distinguishable from that determined by C. More or less dominant over Cr and Ca where distinguishable. (Wright, 1915.) Yellow series — yellowo to creamy. Black series — blacko to sepiay. Brown series — browno (?) to browny. Eye color — black, brown. Cr. Determines the complete absence of yellow, an intensity of dark pigmentation indis- tinguishable from that determined by Cd and an intensity of eye color lower than that determined by C or Cd- More or less dominant over Ca where distinguish- able. (Castle, 1914a; Wright, 1915.) Yellow series — white. Black series — blacko to sepias. Brown series — browno (?) to browny. Eye color — red, brown-red. Ca- Determines an absence of pigment, complete with yellow, not quite complete with dark pigments of the fur and skin, but complete in the eyes. (Castle and Allen, 1903; Castle, 1905; Sollas, 1909; Detlefsen, 1914; Wright, 1915.) Yellow series — white. Black series — white, dark smudges on nose, ears, and feet. Brown series — white, brown smudges on nose, ears, and feet. Eye color — pink. DARK VS. YELLOW COLOR (2). Factors of this group affect skin and fur color, but not eye color. In this group come the factors responsible for self yellows, tortoise-shells, and brindles, on the one hand, and self blacks or browns on the other, as contrasted with the ticked or agouti patterns of the wild rodents. Where more than one factor is present which determines a yellow pattern, combination effects are produced, such as in yellow-spotted agoutis among guinea-pigs. The following factors are known in guinea- pigs: E. A condition for more than a trace of dark pigmentation in the fur; determines dark pig- mentation wherever yellow is not determined by other factors; dominant over e, found in the wild species, all agoutis, blacks, browns, etc., but very rarely in self yellows. e. Determines the presence of one of the yellow colors in all colored areas of the fur, aside from a slight sootiness; responsible for the yellow in most self yellows, for the white in red-eyed whites, etc. (Castle, 1905, 1907, 1907a; Sollas, 1909; Detlefsen, 1914.) A. Determines the presence of a yellow color in the light-bellied agouti pattern wherever there is dark pigmentation in which the yellow group ticking may show; dominant over A' and a, found in Cavia culleri and light-bellied agouti guinea-pigs, includ- ing the red-eyed silver agoutis, in which the agouti pattern is in white. A'. Determines the presence of yellow colors in a more restricted agouti pattern than does A, a pattern usually characterized by a ticked belly not sharply distinct from the sides in color; dominant over a, found in Cavia rufescens and in ticked-bellied agouti hybrids which have rufescens ancestry. (Detlefsen, 1914.) a. Determines the absence of yellow group ticking in hairs of dark pigmentation; found in blacks, browns, etc. (Castle, 1905, 1907, 1907a, 1913; Sollas, 1909; Detlefsen, 1914.) Sj/. An assemblage of unanalyzed factors which determine the presence of spots of a yellow color, conditional on factors of group (1) ; found in black and yellow tortoise-shells, black, yellow, and white tricolors, and in some red-eyed black and white bicolors; probably responsible for an occasional self yellow, though never in the writer's experience. INHERITANCE IN G TINEA-PIGS. \ARIATIONS OF DARK COLOR (3) if this group are responsible for browns and pink-eyed sepias, as compared with blacks, in guinea-pigs; for browns and pink- , yed sepias in mice, and for the new pink-eyed and red-eyed dilute nations in rats. Where more than one factor of this group or of >up 1H determines dilution, combination effects are produced. Thus we have very pale sepias resulting from the combined effects of two independent dilution factors (CdCdPp). B Determines ■ oolor of the black-sepia scries wherever dark pigmentation develops, including the eyes; has no influence where yellow pigmentation develops; domi- nant over b, present in the wild species and in blacks, sepias, albinos with black points, black-eyed yellows, etc. 6. Determines a color of the brown series wherever dark pigmentation develops, including the eyes; has no influence where yellow pigmentation develops; present in browns, brown-eyed yellows, etc. (Castle, 1907a, 1908; Sollas, 1909; Detlefsen, 1914.) /' A inn. lit ion for intense development of dark pigmentation in the fur and for eye colors more intense than pink; not necessary for intense development of yellow; domi- nant over p. p. Determines a low development of dark colors, i. e., below sepias; has no influence where y« How develops; determines pink eye color. (Castle, 1914a.) TABLE OF FACTOR COMBINATIONS. In determining the color which corresponds to a given array of factors the groups of factors must be considered in the order given. Table 33 gives a list of the color varieties corresponding to the combinations of Mendelian factors. At the top and left of the table are indicated, by symbols, the factors present in each of the varieties named in the body of the table. The color of spots produced by 2w and 2y are given below. Only the varieties marked with an asterisk have not yet been synthesized. These include the pink-eyed yellows and creams and a kind of pink-eyed white which is expected to be indistinguishable from an albint > in appearance, though breeding wholly differently. The pink- '1 brown series (bbpp) has not yet been produced and is not included. of the varieties have names given by fanciers which have been used in the literature. In this table, however, it seemed best to use a consistent scheme of naming, indicating at once the color and pattern. Agouti is used as the name for a pattern, the banding of hairs of pre- dominantly a dark color with a yellow color. The names preceding uti give the two colors in each hair. The following table of syno- nym- may lie useful: Black-red agouti*" golden agouti, i-yellow agouti = yellow agouti. ■ •uti = silver agouti. v. ii-red agouti = cinnamon. in agouti = light cinnamon. !>ia =bhie. Brown = chocolate. brown eye (Castle), ruby eye (Sollas). COLOR. Table 33. 67 Factors present. Fur. Eye. EA (agouti light-belly). EA' (agouti ticked-belly). Eaa. ee (A, A' or aa). BPC CdCd.. CdCr... CdCa.. CrCr... CrCa- • • CaCa • ■ BppC... CdCd . . CdCr. . . CdCa.. CrCr... Black-red agouti Dark sepia-yellow agouti Dark sepia-cream agouti Black Dark sepia Do Red Yellow Cream . . Black. Do. Do. Do. Red. Do. Pink. Pink. Do. Do. Do. Do. Do. Do. Brown. Do. Do. Do. Brown-red. Do. Pink. Dark sepia-white agouti Light sepia-white agouti White (dark points) Light sepia Do Dark sepia Light sepia White (light points) . Do. . White (dark points). Pale sepia Verv pale sepia . Do .Do. Pale sepia-red agouti Very pale sepia-yellow agouti . Verv pale sepia-cream agouti. . Red * Yellow *Cream . . Do Do . . .*Do Very pale sepia-white agouti . . Do ....Do Do * White . .*Do CaOa . • bbPC CdCd . . CaCr... CdCa. . CrCr... CrCa... CaCa- - • White (light points) White (light points). Brown . . .*Do Brown-red agouti Red. . Medium brown-yellow agouti . Medium brown-cream agouti . Light brown-cream agouti .... Medium brown-white agouti . . Light brown-white agouti .... Medium brown . ....Do Yellow Cream Do . . Medium brown . Light brown .... White Do White (It. br. points) . Do Factors present. Sw. 2y Sw2y. Eye. C CdCd.. CdCr... CdCa.. CrCr- - . CrCa... White spots (clear) Red spots Yellow spots . . . Cream spots .... Do Red and white tri- color. Yellow and white. Cream and white tricolor. Do .... Do .... Do Do ...Do Do 1 White spots, / often sooty. (Albino) Sooty and clear, white spots. (Albino) C aCa- • • (Albino) HEREDITARY FACTORS AND THE PHYSIOLOGY OF PIGMENT. The definitions which have been given for the hereditary factors are based largely on the colors as seen without a microscope. It would be very desirable, however, to correlate color factors accurately with the variations in quality and quantity of the actual pigments and ultimately with the physiology and chemistry of pigment formation. Considerable progress has been made in recent years in the study of the chemistry of melanin pigments. The melanins are amorphous granular pigments found throughout the animal kingdom. A large number of researches have established the fact that substances which closely resemble the natural melanins can be produced by the action of ((S QfHBBITANCl IN GUINEA-PIGS. MrUin oxidising enzymes on tyrosin and related aromatic compounds. rosiD is in unportanl constituent of protein molecules and there is much reason to believe that tyrosin and related substances are the chromogens from which the natural melanins are formed. Tyrosinase, .Ul enaione whirl, can oxidize tyrosin to dark substances resembling melanins, has been found very widely among animals, including the INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. produoed at a lower rate of production of I than is the case of I alone. Uwve the level al which I alone produces yellow, the two kinds of eniymes, yellow- and black-producing, compete with each other for chromogen, producing a mixture of black and yellow, the relative importance depending on the rate at which II is produced. Because of the competition the intensity of black shows two maxima as production increases— one just below the yellow threshold and the other al maximal production of I. Intensity of production or inhibi- tion <>f II in patterns in the fur are determined by various factors group 2) which produce self yellow, yellow spotting, agouti, etc. There is a third group of substances which, added to the dark- pigment-producing enzyme (II), affect the intensity of dark color pro- duced hut not the power of fixing chromogen in competition with the yellow producing enzyme. They have no effect on the intensity of yellow. In this group are the brown factors of mice and guinea-pigs, and perhaps rabbits and dogs, the pink-eye factor of rats, mice, and guinea-pigs and the new red-eye factor of rats, i.e., the factorsof group 3. While based to a larger extent on the genetic facts in the albino Beriee in guinea-pigs, the hypothesis explains many cases in other mammals in the sense that apparently complex variations are reduced to a single physiological cause. In rabbits, single Mendelian factors produce some rather complex variations. A single factor changes a self black to the gray color with a Yrllmv-ticked back but a pure white belly. Another variation changes a self black to a sooty yellow with a black belly. These varia- tions combined in one animal give a white-bellied clear yellow. How can each of these apparently complex color changes be determined by a simple physiological change? Let us suppose that in all rabbits I is produced strongly on the back, but so feebly on the belly that it is below the yellow threshold, but not so feebly that black is greatly affected. Let us suppose that II is likewise more strongly produced on back than on belly. A factor which tends to produce an inhibitor <»f II is added. On the back II (the black-producing enzyme) is inhibited in only a portion of the development of the hair, leaving yellow ticking. On the belly all II is inhibited, leaving white. The result is a gray rabbit. The other factor causes a general slowing up in the production of II. On the back this enables the yellow-pro- ducing enzyme to predominate in competition and sooty yellow results. On the belly— below the yellow threshold — what little black-producing enzyme does develop has no competition and only black can result. We p;et a black-bellied sooty yellow. The combination pattern can only he a white-bellied yellow. In many other mammals color phases ar»- found which can be explained as due either to variations in production of II or I. The red phase of the red fox has a white che The level of production of I is below the yellow threshold COLOR. 73 but above the black threshold on the chest. Increase in production of II produces the silver phase, nearly self sepia in color, including the chest. The colors of the varying hare seem to be due to variations in production of I determined by environmental causes. The white winter pelage gives way in blotches to a white-ticked sepia; this gives way to yellow-ticked sepia as the intensity of production of the basic enzyme rises above the yellow threshold and in some varieties the full summer pelage is almost self red. Many other cases could be given in which two color phases of an individual animal or the color patterns of closely allied varieties seem to differ in many respects and yet can be explained on the basis outlined as due to a single physiological change. In the case of very complex color patterns, it is necessary to suppose that the power of producing the hypothetical enzymes I, II, or III may be distributed in quite complex patterns. But the hypothesis often gives a simple explanation for certain peculiarities in a pattern. In the tiger, the stripes on the back are quite intense yellow and black. The yellow stripes grow paler down the sides, becoming white on the lower sides and belly. The black stripes likewise grow lighter down the sides but at the point at which the yellow becomes white, the black stripes suddenly grow more intense, at least in some individuals, to become paler again on the belly. Again, on the legs, which are white on the inside, yellow on the outside, black stripes are visible on the white part, but either disappear completely or leave merely a streak of sooty red on the yellow part. All of this becomes intelligible if we assume that the basic enzyme (I) is produced at decreasing rates from back to belly and from outside to inside of leg, while the black-produc- ing supplement (II) is distributed in vertical stripes (horizontal on the legs). Two parallel stripes give a remarkable reproduction of the variation in black and yellow in the albino series in guinea-pigs. We have the same change from black and intense yellow to sepia and cream, then to darker sepia and white, and finally light sepia and white, illustrating the different thresholds for the appearance of black and yellow and the reduction in intensity of black above the yellow threshold due to the entrance of competition at this point. DISCUSSION OF EXPERIMENTS. MATERIAL. SYSTEMATIC POSITION. Guinea-pigB belong to the family Caviidae of the hystricomorph division of rodents. There arc throe living genera of Caviidae: Doli- I teem., which contains the large Patagoniancavies; Hydrochcerus i. to which belongs the capybara; and Cavia Pallas, containing the small cavies. Genus Cavia is divided into two subgenera, Cavia proper and Cerodon V. Cuv., distinguished most conspicuously by the greater complexity of the molars in the former. Seven living species are listed under Cavia proper by Trouessart (1904): C. rufescens Lund, a small dark Brazilian cavy with subspecies in Guiana and Argentina. c. fvlgida Wagler, a Brazilian cavy probably closely allied to rufescens (Thomas, 1901). C. aperea End., B large pale-colored Brazilian cavy. ( . aeara Wagner, a cavy of Paraguay probably closely alhed to aperea (Thomas, 1901). C. mthri Bennett, a small pale-oolored cavy of Peru. ( \ tschudii Fitzinger, a large, richly colored cavy described from lea, Peru. C. inrcellus Linn., the tame guinea-pig, much larger than at least rvfescens and cutleri. DESCRIPTION OF STOCKS. 1 . >ur of these species are dealt with in the experiments to be described, viz, Cavia rufescens, C. cutleri, C. porcellus, and a type which is quite certainly that described as C. tschudii, although it is also quite certain that it is simply feral porcellus. Breeding experiments have been carried on with a fifth species, C. aperea, by Nehring (1894). The C. rufescens stock was derived from 3 individuals received from Mr. Adolph Hempel, of Campinas, Brazil, in 1903. The history of tins stock is fully described by Detlefsen (1914). When received by the writer, most of the stock consisted of hybrids containing only from • rufescens blood. There were a few § and \ bloods and one ^ blood, 9 A68, which is still alive (August 1915) at the remarkable age of S years 1 month,1 a good illustration of the vigor of the first-generation hybrids. All of the pure rufescens stock has died out. The rufescens hybrids have been crossed with nearly all of the guinea-pig stocks to be described, and most of the color varieties may be found among them. The ticked-bellied type of agouti has been found only among them and in pure rufescens. c. rufescens was not completely fertile with the guinea-pigs Detlefsen, 1914). Detlefsen found that while the female hybrids were fertile, all of the male hybrids obtained were sterile. In the 1 rufescens, derived by crossing the females with guinea-pigs, the males were again all Bterile. Not until the |- bloods were obtained did I ■ • ■! I >• tot* r L915, aged 8 years, :; months. — W. E. C. MATERIAL. 75 a few fertile males appear. The percentage of fertile males gradually increased in later generations. The Cavia cutleri stock was derived from animals captured by Pro- fessor Castle in Peru in 1911 . Like C. rufescens, these are much smaller than the guinea-pig. All show the agouti pattern. The color is described on page 59. Unlike C. rufescens, C. cutleri breeds freely in captivity and crosses readily with the guinea-pig. The male and female hybrids are fertile. The lea stock of guinea-pigs was derived from 3 guinea-pigs which were obtained by Castle near lea, Peru, in 1911. They were as large as or larger than average guinea-pigs, and of a rich golden agouti color, very different from C. cutleri. Two independent color variations appeared at once in the pure stock, viz, black (aa) and red-eye (CrCr). Such variations are very uncommon among wild species of animals; e. g.. none has occurred within the pure rufescens or cutleri stocks. Both of these variations are found in domesticated guinea-pigs in Peru (Arequipa stock). From the description of Cavia tschudii, quoted in Waterhouse (1848) under the name C. cutleri Tschudi, it seems clear that our lea stock is the same as the former, which was likewise described from lea. In view, however, of the size, color, and possession of recessive color varieties found among tame guinea-pigs of Peru, there can be little doubt that they are feral porcellus. The Arequipa stock comes from a pair of guinea-pigs brought from Arequipa, Peru, by Castle in 1911. He obtained them from Indians who had them under domestication. Owing to the early death of the only female, no pure stock could be developed, but numerous descen- dants have been derived from the original male 1002, a sepia-cream agouti with white and cream spots, demonstrated to be of constitution EEA%BBPpCdCr, and from a son of the original pair, male 1007, a yellow agouti with white and yellow spots, demonstrated to be of con- stitution EeAaBBPPCdCd. These were crossed mainly with the 4-toe and BW stocks, which are described below. For a full discussion of the origin and nature of the pure cutleri, lea, and Arequipa stocks, see Part I. The Lima stock comes from 8 guinea-pigs obtained from Indians near Lima, Peru, by Professor Brues in 1913. These guinea-pigs and their descendants have only recently been crossed with other stocks. There have been no agoutis in this stock. The pink-eye and yellow variations, as well as white spotting (but not yellow spotting), have occurred in this stock. A pink-eyed red, the lowest recessive, of this stock is of constitution eeaaBBppCC . There were both rough-furred and smooth- furred individuals in the original stock. The following stocks come from guinea-pigs obtained from fanciers by Professor Castle and have been maintained for several years at the Bussey Institution. 76 INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PKJS. SB Block. A Mock consisting exclusively of very intense blacks. So red or white Bpotting h;ls been observed among them. Unfortu- nately it is a stock of low fortility, and could not be used much to advantage . /.Ml' gtodk.— This stock has for years consisted exclusively of very intense blacks and very Booty albinos. The blacks occasionally show i few red hairs or a small red patch. This has been an extremely ful stock, among other thingB, furnishing albinos known to be geneti- cally identical with blacks, except for the albino factor. (Race B of ,r-tn< slock — This is a much-inbred stock, practically all the indi- viduals of which show four good toes on the hind feet instead of the normal three. This stock was developed by selection and inbreeding by Professor ( !astle (Castle, 1906). Most of the individuals are a dull black with dull red blotching and brindling and often with white spots. Albinos appear quite frequently and reds much more rarely. Table 34. — Genetic formulae of stocks. • k. Color. Roughness. Mcndelian. Unanalyzed. Men- delian. Unana- lyzed. ( :ivia cutleri ia rufeacens .... Ir:i E \ B P C E A' B P C E A, a B P C,Cr E,e A,a B P,p C.Cd.Cr E.e a B P,p C E a B P C E a B P C,Ca E(e)a B P C.Ca E a B,b P C E.e a B P Cd,Ca e a b P Cd.Ca E,e A.A'.aB.b P C,Cd,Cr,Ca (2w2y) 2int + 2w2y 2int + 2w 2int + (2y) 2int + 2w2y 2int - 2w2y (2w2y) 2int - 2int- 2w2y 2int± r S r S r S R,r s R,r (S)s r s r s R,r s R,r S,s r s r s R,r S,s 2- .... 2- .... 2R .... 2R 2- .... 2- .... 2R 2+ .... Arrquipa Lima BW 4-toe Tricolor • ia+cream Brown-eyed cream . . ' rufeaoena hybrids. / n ih, tricolor stock the fur is typically a patchwork of red, white, and black. Full-roughs, partial-roughs, and smooths occur among them. The writer has used many guinea-pigs of very mongrel ancestry, which, however, owe their partial rough coat to this stock. Tl i i picHmdrcn am and brown-eyed cream stocks have been selected for years for extreme dilution. The former stock consists exclusively of Bepias, Mack-eyed yellows and creams, and albinos. The latter siste exclusively of brown-eyed yellows and creams and albinos. C of Part I.) In the tables, these together are called dilute- Belection -tuck. Table :;t shows the Mcndelian factors affecting color and roughness of fur which occur in each stock. Unanalyzed hereditary conditions which affed color and roughness are also included, prefixed by the symbol I. 2w and 2y. as has already been stated, mean hereditary INHERITANCE OF DILUTION. 77 white and yellow spotting respectively. 2 int+ and 2 int— mean hereditary constitutions which intensify or dilute, respectively, the color associated with a given array of Mendelian factors. 2 + and 2 — in the rough column have a similar meaning with respect to the rough character. 2R means the presence of roughness of a different kind from that analyzed. Where a factor occurs only rarely in a stock, it is inclosed in parentheses. PROBLEMS. The inheritance of the discontinuous color variations which are known in guinea-pigs has been solved by previous work. After each factor variation from the wild type (Cavia cutleri) in the definitions of the factors the principal papers on the subject are given. The writer has been concerned mainly with an analysis of inheritance in the contin- uous series of variations by which each of the intense colors — red, brown, and black — grade into dilute colors and ultimately white. A second group of problems concerns the variations in the amount of yellow ticking in agoutis. The writer has worked with the agouti patterns of Cavia cutleri, C. rufescens hybrids, and tame guinea-pigs. The inheritance of variations in the rough coat occasionally found in guinea-pigs is discussed in a later section. INHERITANCE OF DILUTION. THE RED-EYE FACTOR. The experiments with dilution have become closely associated with experiments with certain imported South American stocks (lea, Are- quipa) which are discussed in detail in Part I. A number of hitherto unknown color varieties appeared in these stocks, the inheritance of which could be explained by assuming the existence of a new allelo- morph of albinism intermediate in effect and dominance between albin- ism and its normal allelomorph. More specifically, this new factor is characterized by the production of red eyes, slight dilution of black in the fur, and complete inhibition of yellow pigment development. The writer has used animals of both the lea and Arequipa stocks in experiments, with results in full agreement with those given in Part I. Crosses 20-1 and 21 to 25 involve red-eye (from lea stock) without also involving dilution. In cross 20-1 a pure lea male, a red-eyed agouti, is crossed with intense guinea-pigs, giving young all intense. This illustrates the dominance of intensity over red-eye. In cross 21 a pure lea intense male crossed with albinos of intense stock gives both intense and red-eye young. The lea male no doubt was heterozygous for red-eye, but the albinos could not possibly trans- mit red-eye, as they come from a stock in which red-eye has never appeared. This illustrates the apparent reversal of dominance of red- eye whenever albinism is introduced into a cross. A further illus- tration is given in cross 23, in which red-eye by albino of intense 7s IMIKKIIA.V K IN GUINEA-PIGS. ok gives red-eyes, hut no intense young. In cross 25, red-eyes -. ,1 with jilhinos from various sources give no intense young, but only red-eyes and albinos. One possible explanation of these results would be the supposition that red-eye becomes dominant over its normal allelomorph iii the presence of heterozygous albinism. In this case intense young should appear when such heterozygous red-eyes are crossed together; but, as is shown in cross 24, none such appears. Here red-eyes from cross 21, mated inter se, gave 17 red-eyes, 6 albinos, DO intense. Numerous results of this kind have made it clear that intensity can never be recovered in any generation after a cross of red- eye wit h albino. This means that neither red-eye nor albino can trans- mit the normal allelomorph of the other. Now, the one thing which a recessive variation, of necessity, can not transmit, is its own normal allelomorph. Therefore the normal allelomorphs of red-eye and albino must be identical. This does not yet demonstrate that albinism, red-eye, and intensity form a series of three allelomorphs. There is still the possibility that red-eve and albinism involve the same recessive allelomorph (Ca) of norma] color (C), but differ by an independent modifying factor. Symbolically we could suppose albinos to be CaCarr, red-eyes to be 1 I IvR (or CaCaEr), intense guinea-pigs of ordinary stocks to be CCrr (or C '( Jarr) , and intense guinea-pigs of lea stock to be CCRR (or CCaRR) . We must suppose the lea stock to be homozygous for the modifier R, to account for the absence of albinos. R must be a unit factor to account for the simple 3 to 1 ratio in cross 24. This hypothesis fits all of the facts given so far. The critical test of its truth is the possibility (as it turns out, impossibility) of producing intense animals (CCaRr) which will give both red-eyes and albinos when crossed with albinos. If intensity, red-eye, and albinism are triple allelomorphs, it should be impossible to obtain such animals. Crosses 21 and 22 are interesting as furnishing just this test. Cross 21 may be represented symbolically as follows according to the two hypotheses: •w (BW) X intense (lea) = 9 intense + 4 red-eye. (1) CaCarr X CCaRR = CCaRr CaCaRr. (2) CaCa X CCr = CCa CrCa. In oit her case the F, red-eyes crossed inter se should give 3 red-eyes to 1 albino. The result obtained in cross 24 (17 red-eyes to 6 albinos) is in nearly perfect agreement. But the cross of Fx intense with albinos gives very different results under the two hypotheses (cross 22): -"' X iniensi r tu multiple factors, or even whether its inheritance is Mendelian or not, provided only that it is inherited independently of albinism. J tosses 16 and 17 and table 35 give the actual results. All cases are included, which involve an intense stock known to carry no dilution. Among those called dilute below (among the young), none was more intense than sepia* or yellow4. Among the intense, none was more dilute than a dull black comparable in grade but not in color with sepia2, or a red in very few if any cases as dilute as yellow2. There was there- lore no difficulty in drawing a natural line between intense and dilute in these crosses. INHERITANCE OF DILUTION. 81 It is evident that the two sets of crosses give consistently different results. This difference demonstrates that dilution does not segregate independently of albinism. An even more striking result follows from a portion of the above data. F: dilutes, one of whose parents was of intense stock, were back-crossed with albinos of intense stock. They gave 9 dilute, 20 albino young, no intense, although these young were at least three-quarters of intense stock. On the other hand, Fi intense, one of whose parents was an albino of dilute stock, were back-crossed with albinos of dilute stock. They gave 5 intense, 7 albinos, no dilutes, although these young were at least three-quarters of dilute stock. It is clear that the hereditary difference between a dilute and an intense can not be transmitted through an albino. Table 35. Intense. Dilute. Red-eyed. White. cT albino (intense stock) X 9 dilute 56 29 5 10 39 21 9 albino (intense stock) X cf dilute Total 85 15 60 d* albino (dilute stock) X 9 intense (intense stock) . . 9 albino (dilute stock) X cfintense (intense stock) . . 47 9 10 2 56 12 It was emphasized above that all the intense animals used in cross 17 came from stocks which have never given dilutes. This was necessary because in other crosses (18, 34, 41) intense by albino has given many dilute young. No such precaution was taken with the dilutes used in cross 16. Any available dilutes were used regardless of ancestry. In fact, 1 1 of them, with 38 young, had one or both parents intense. In none of the other crosses in which dilute has been crossed with albino (19, 27, 38, 44) has any intense young appeared. Thus in crosses with albinos an intense may transmit dilution, but a dilute never trans- mits intensity. From these crosses it seems clear that intensity is dom- inant over dilution. Other crosses on the whole bear this out. The apparent exceptions will be ignored for the present but discussed later. We have reached the definite conclusion that dilute by albino can never give intense, regardless of ancestry on either side. Since the only thing which a variety of necessity can not transmit is a dominant allelomorph of its essential factor, it follows that dilution and albinism must have the same dominant allelomorph, which we will call intensity. There are only a few hypotheses which will satisfy this condition. We already know two recessive allelomorphs of intensity, viz, red-eye and albinism. It is conceivable that dilution may be due to the cooperation of an independent factor (or factors) with one or more of the known combinations CrCr, CrCa, and CaCa. If this is not the case, S> INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. dilution must be due to a new allelomorph in the albino series, let us v.lV i \ modifying factor which shows partial coupling would give intermediate results. 1 Since dilution and red-eye show considerable resemblance, it would be a plausible hyp.. thesis to assume that they are due to the same allelomorph in the albino series (Cp) but differ by an independent modi- fying factor (D) . With this hypothesis, all stocks used (except the lea and Arequipa) must needs be homozygous for the modifier in order that no red-eyes should appear. Dilutes would be CrCrDD or CrCaDD albinos C.C.DD in these stocks. Thus albinos of these stocks should transmit the modifier and in crosses with red-eyes (CrCrdd) should produce dilutes at least in F2. But in crosses 23 and 25, red-eyes mated with such albinos have given no dilutes, nor have dilutes appeared in Fj in cross 24, among 23 young. Thus an albino can not transmit the hereditary difference between a dilute and a red-eye and the hypothesis is untenable. (2) Next to be considered is the hypothesis that there is a modifier which converts into a dilute an animal which would otherwise be an all )ino. Dilutes of ordinary stock would be CaCaDD or CaCaDd. In cross 20, dilutes of ordinary stock crossed wdth a pure lea male No. 724, a homozygous red-eye (CrCrdd), produced 5 dilute young which must be of formula CrCaDd. This shows that if the hypothesis is to stand at all, it must be extended, so that the factor which converts an albino into a dilute also converts a red-eye into a dilute. The fact ih.it a dilute may transmit red-eye (crosses 19 and 27) is further evi- dence that this extension is necessary. In this form most of the results can be explained satisfactorily. (3) The only other hypothesis wmich remains is that dilution is due to a new allelomorph in the albino series making a series of four — C, Cd, Cr, and Ca. The results cited above (crosses 20, 19, and 27) make it evident that dilution is dominant over red-eye. The meaning of a Beries of four allelomorphs can be made clear by considering all of the possible zygotic formulae. Every zygote must have two representatives from the series, but never more than two. Intense guinea-pigs may be homozygous (< C), or carry dilution (CCd), or red-eye (CCr), or albin- ism ('Ca), but can never transmit more than one of the recessive conditions. Dilutes may be homozygous (CciCci) or carry red-eye or albinism (CdCa), never both. Red-eyes may be homozygous or carry albinism (CrCa), while albinos can only be homozygous 1 and can never transmit any of the higher conditions. The critical t est between this hypothesis of four allelomorphs and the preceding one (that dilute is a modified red-eye or albino), lies in the possibility or impossibility of producing animals which in crosses with all linos will transmit more than one recessive condition. If an intense animal can be obtained which transmits both dilution and red-eye INHERITANCE OF DILUTION. 83 (CCrDd) or dilution and albinism (CCJDd), or if a dilute can be obtained which transmits both red-eye and albinism (CrCaDd), the hypothesis of modifiers must be adopted. But all attempts to obtain these double heterozygotes have failed. All of the results substantiate the hypothesis of quadruple allelomorphs. Arequipa male No. 1007 was of formula CrCrDD or CdCd, depending on the hypothesis chosen (see crosses 28 to 34). He was crossed with intense guinea-pigs of BW or 4-toe stock, known to transmit no dilu- tion (CCadd or CCJ . The intense young could only be CCrDd or CCd under the two hypotheses. Five of them were crossed with albinos and gave 13 intense, 20 dilute young, no others (cross 34). Expectation on the hypothesis of a modifier is 16 intense, 8 dilute, 8 red-eye. On the hypothesis of allelomorphs it is 16 intense to 16 dilute. Both the excess of dilutes and the absence of red-eyes point conclusively to the latter. In cross 18, intense guinea-pigs, each of which had a dilute parent known to transmit albinism and with no lea or Arequipa blood, are crossed with albinos or red-eyes. Under the modifier hypothesis we would expect about half of them to be CCaDd. Under the allelomorph hypothesis, they should be CCd or CCa. As it turned out, there were 6 which gave only intense and dilute (30 intense, 35 dilute) and 8 which gave no dilute young (57 intense to 61 red-eye or albino). Thus there was no intense which had dilute young and also red-eyes or albinos. This result distinctly favors the hypothesis of allelomorphs. In crosses 19 and 27 dilutes, each from the cross of a red-eye with a stock guinea-pig free from South American ancestry, are crossed with albinos. Under the modifier hypothesis, those which transmit red-eye at all are necessarily CrCaDd, for they must be CraCraD in order to appear dilute; they could get Ca, but not Cr, from the stock guinea-pig parent, and they would necessarily get d from the red-eye parent. Under the allelomorph hypothesis, they should be CdCr, the rest CdCa; 9 gave only dilutes and red-eyes (18 dilutes, 24 red-eyes); 9 others gave only dilutes and albinos (20 dilutes, 16 albinos). There were 3 which had had only 8 dilute young when tabulated. The fact that none of the 9 which had red-eye young also had albinos among 42 young gives a third body of evidence pointing toward the allelomorph hypothesis. These results make it reasonably certain that the allelomorph hypo- thesis is correct. The only other possibility would involve coupling so close as to simulate multiple allelomorphs. The hypothesis of allelo- morphs has been reached by a method of elimination. It remains to show that all of the data are in harmony with it. In the next section, definite conclusions are reached as to the inheritance of variations in intensity and dilution which make it possible to distinguish intense animals from dilute in all but very exceptional cases. M INHERITANCE IN CUINEA-PIGS. The following table gives a summary of the data bearing on the inheritance of the albino series of allelomorphs based on these conclu- gion8 n will be noticed that animals of every possible formula have been tested by crosses with albinos, the lowest recessives. No attempt has been made bo make all other possible crosses, and several (especially Tabu 86, -Summary of albino series crosses (crosses 16 to 44). Parents. Int. dm x albino. Dilute X albino. \,- \ albino Albino X albino. . Intense x red-eye Dilute x dilute Intense x intense Formulas. X CaCa. • • < '( (I CaCa- • ■ CCr CaCa- • • CCa CaCa- • • I il' d X CaCa- • ■ ( VlC'r CaCa- • • CdCa CaCa- • • CrCr X CaCa- CrCa CaCa • Int. 40 31 9 64 Dilute X red-eye. . Red-eye X red-eye Intense X dilute Ca^a X va^a- • • °* {§£}■■ °* {§§}• CdCa CrCr. CrCa. CrCa X CrCa- CC X CdCd. CC CdCa. CCd CdCd. CCd CdCa. . CC., CdCd- CCa CdC a. . Dil. R.E. 36 26 18 98 9 28 25 CdCdX CdCa. CCd X CCd • • - ('<',1 CCa.... CC CC . . . . 14 12 10 32 12 28 31 15 1 13 24 30 6 W. 71 20 57 75 X i 40 10 22 15 82 19 35 1 5 17 79 3 X From crosses- 15 24 17a, 6. 186, 34, 41 21 17c, 17d, 18c, 22 16a, 38a, 44 19,27 166, 16c, 19, 27, 33, 386, 44 25 23,25 Long established. 20 18a, 20 18c 20, 26, 43 20 26,43 24 28 35 29, 40a 32, 36, 40a 28, 40a 36, 406 30, 37, 42 37,42 31,39 39 See rough and Lima crosses ones involving red-eye) have not yet been made by the writer. The Lasl column refers to the crosses tabulated at the end of the paper. The ratio- expected are obvious from the nature of the matings, except that 39 to 11 were not random crosses of their kind. The appearance of recessive young was used as a criterion of the nature of the parents in these cases. This causes an excess of recessives to be expected. INHERITANCE OF MINOR VARIATIONS IN INTENSITY. 85 INHERITANCE OF MINOR VARIATIONS IN INTENSITY. METHODS AND ACCURACY OF GRADING. The method of grading has been described on page 60. E very- guinea-pig which showed dilute black or yellow in the fur was compared with standard samples of hair within a week of birth. These samples were black0, sepia3, sepia6, and sepia9, in the black series, and red0, yellow.3, and cream6 in the yellow series. Intermediate grades were given by estimate. Grades were taken later in life in many cases in order to determine the relation of age to intensity of pigmentation. In interpreting the results, it is important to know the accuracy with which the grading could be done and the difficulties met. In some cases the back and belly are fairly uniform in intensity, but usually the belly is considerably the lighter. Tufts of hair for grading have always been taken as near the middle of the back as possible. In some cases the hair is of fairly uniform intensity from base to tip. In most cases, however, the base is very much lighter than the tip. The color at the tip has been used in grading, although extreme varia- tions in the intensity at the base have also been noted. The color at the tip has most to do with the general appearance of the animal. The attempt has been made to get both a yellow and a sepia grade for every animal, so that the correlation between the intensities in these series could be determined. This is easy in the sepia and yellow-spotted animals, but in agoutis (where the yellow band of the agouti pattern displaces the sepia near the tip of the hair) determination of the inten- sity of sepia has not been so satisfactory. Several independent determi- nations have been taken in many of these cases. In most cases the same grade was assigned the second time and rarely did the second grade differ from the first by more than one point. VARIATIONS IN INTENSE GUINEA-PIGS AND ALBINOS. Before discussing the inheritance of variations among dilutes, it will be well to note briefly the range of variation among guinea-pigs which have the intensity factors (CP). In the BW race the blacks are a very intense black. The base of the hair is only slightly lighter than the tip. In other races, especially the 4-toe stock, the tip of the hair is a dull slaty black and the base a very dull color, often with less pigment than many typical dilutes. The animals have a dull streaky black appearance very different usually from the uniform dark sepia of the darker dilutes. This dull color is not associated with heterozygous albinism. Male M330 was undoubtedly homozygous (CC), having had 9 intense young by albino females and no others; yet he was one of the dullest blacks in stock. On the other hand, nearly all of the intense blacks of the BW race are heterozygous for albinism. 86 INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. This (U1" black can not be due to an allelomorph of albinism between intensity C) and dilution (C(1), since it is a condition which can be tr:m>! i lit ted by albinos. Indeed, the albinos themselves of the BW and 4-toe Btocks differ conspicuously in appearance. The BW albinos have je1 black ears and feet, dark smudges on the nose, and usually M„ on the back. The 4-toe and most other albinos (at the Bussey Institution I have very much less black on ears, nose, and feet, and the rest of the fur is pure white. There are parallel variations in the intensity of red in these stocks. 'The occasional red spots in the BW race are of a very intense red standard redo). In the 4-toe and other dull stocks the red is consider- ably less intense, especially at the base of the hair. The most dilute grade found in tame guinea-pigs known to have factor C is yellow2 D12 cross 35-1). The wild Cavia cutleri is quite light in color. The black of the fur is a dull slaty color, more like the dull black of the 4-toes than any other color in tame guinea-pigs. The yellow on the back is about yellow3, on the belly cream^ In spite of the resemblance to tame yellow agoutis, Covin cutleri has the intensity and not the dilution factor. When crossed with animals of the BW race, whether blacks or albinos, the young are intermediate in intensity and would be called intense (Part I). Crossed with black animals of the 4-toe race, the young are but little more intense than Cavia cutleri. (See plate 3.) Summing up : All variations maybe found among intense guinea-pigs, from uniform black0 to a dull slaty black2 and from red0 to yellow2. In the dull grades the hair is especially dull at the base. These variations are hereditary, but have not been analyzed. The hereditary factors for these variations in intense guinea-pigs are responsible for visible differences among albinos. It is to be expected, as indeed is the case, that variations will be found among dilutes, for which these same unanalyzed hereditary differences of different stocks are responsible. Finally, the residual heredity of all tame guinea-pigs has more intensi- fying effect than that of Cavia cutleri, the wild species. MULTIPLE ALLELOMORPHS. The ] presence of at least four allelomorphs in the albino series suggests the hypothesis that other allelomorphs in the series may be responsible for the intermediate grades in intensity. It is a tempting hypothesis suppose that the continuous series of variations is correlated with a continuous Beries of allelomorphs, such that each grade of intensity is dominant over all lower grades. If this were the case a stock of dilute-, in which all derive their dilution from a single gamete of one animal, should he fairly constant in their degree of dilution. Again, the cross of dilute by dilute should never give young more intense than the darker parent. INHERITANCE OF MINOR VARIATIONS IN INTENSITY. 87 However, both of these tests fail. No single gamete stock of dilutes has been found which will not give the entire range of variation when tested. Thus, male D30 red0, an intense which carried dilution as a recessive (CCd), was crossed with red-eyes. His dilute young must all owe their dilution to the same single gamete. They ranged from D340 blackx to D152 sepia5. Yellows which owe their dilution to this same single gamete (derived from male 00 cream6 CdCa, the father of D30) range from D391 yellow2 to 00 cream^. Dilution from a single gamete of A674 sepia6 (CdCa) has given rise to D652 blacki and M306 sepia7, D409 yellow3, and Ml 99 cream7. This last case involves no admixture of South American blood. Inspection of the tables will yield many similar cases. Evidently dilution from a single gamete may appear in dilutes of any grade of intensity. The extreme variations may occur within a single litter (offspring of D30). Again, many examples can be given in which the offspring are much darker than either parent. D652 blacki was the offspring of D215 sepia3 and D106 sepia4. In cross 37 there are 6 cases in which cream6 X cream6 has produced yellow3, with other less extreme cases of this kind. These results do not demonstrate that no more than four allelomorphs in the albino series are present in our stock. They do show that there are other causes producing varia- tion of much more importance than any other allelomorphs which may be present. THE RELATIONS OF IMPERFECT DOMINANCE, STOCK, AND AGE TO GRADES OF INTENSITY. In tables 37 and 38, and diagrammatically in figure 5 and figure 6, all records of grades of dilution at birth are analyzed with respect to genetic constitution and stock. All of those whose genetic constitu- tion was known with complete or nearly complete certainty, either from parentage or from offspring, are put after the proper formula, CdCd, CdCr, etc. All from litters containing two classes are listed separately with the numerical expectation of the classes as (20 CdCd: 32 CdCa), etc. Those in the litters whose formulae were later determined by a test mating are given below in parentheses. These tested individuals are included both among those of certain constitution and in the litters containing two classes. No very close analysis of the influence of stock was possible from the data obtained. However, the following stocks were recognized : Dil., Dilute selection stock. Misc., Miscellaneous stocks with but little BW blood and no lea or Arequipa blood. These contained much dilute selection and 4-toe blood and some C. rufescens ancestry. hBW, Fx from the cross of miscellaneous with BW stock. \BW, Back-cross of iBW with BW stock. S. Am., All animals with lea or Arequipa blood, in most cases about \ South American, \ BW, and | miscellaneous, but including pure lea, IcaXBW, etc. ^ IMM.KITANCE IN GUINEA-PKiS. In each array Of animals of known constitution and stock the number of animals involved, the mean grade of dilution, and the standard deviation of the frequency polygon are given. It will be noticed that the Btandard deviations decrease as the analysis is made closer. For Red White -Variations in intensity of yellow. Formula and stock printed near mode of each distribution. mple, the >tandard deviation for all dilute blacks is 1.53, for dilute blacks of formula CdCa is 1.13, and for those of formula CdCa and of South American stock is 1.02. The corresponding numbers for dilute \ellows are 1.10, 0.76, and 0.59, respectively. INHERITANCE OF MINOR VARIATIONS IN INTENSITY. 89 In tables 39 and 40 are given the mean grades at birth and when more than 4 months old for all guinea-pigs which were graded these two times. These data are arranged by constitution and stock. In most cases the mean grade at birth of the sample graded twice agrees well with the mean grade at birth of the whole array of the same constitu- tion and stock. Black0 Sep, Sep Fig. 6. — Variations in intensity of dilute blacks. Formula and stock printed near mode of each distribution. VARIATIONS OF YELLOW. I owe the suggestion that heterozygous albinism may be correlated with extreme dilution of yellow to Professor Castle, who found that attempts to select for a cream stock of maximum dilution led to stocks which invariably gave numerous albinos. The tables and figures con- firm this suggestion in a very striking way. Animals known to be homozygous dilute (CdCd) vary between yellow2 and yellow4 with the mean at yellow2.9. Those known to transmit albinism vary between QO INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. I uftU W KjJicts of stock and imperfed dominance on intensity of yellow. list it tit inn. Stork. Redg Red] V2 Yi Y4 Cr4 o J 6 I >•< 3 \ in ! to 4 14 23 7 9 27 20 (3) (4) 13 19 < \K\i ihi 21 25 4 i to 1 33 l to 'BW l to ;b\y I to S.Am 3 1 1 4 .< r 15 CdC. ( ',!( 'r bv tc-t Do .Do. . ( '.!< \ I'V t08t , .Do 12 ( ',!< ',1' 23 CdCa Dil . 9 20 (2) 12 18 • lCd:43CdC». ( ,n ',i t'v teal Miso 3 (1) 5 4 (1) 5 (2) !>,, by teal .Do. . (4) m (2) lCd:3.5CdCa. 1 MCl S.Am.BW. 3 14 4 9 36 70 3 Total 7 25 13 169 335 2.88 4.31 5.12 4.72 .65 .46 .76 1.10 6 i . . . . . .Do 4 80 . . .Do 49 4 7 1 34 10 41 I), I . ..Do 9 44 128 77 Tahle 38. — Effects of stock and ■ Imperfect dominana. : on intensity of black. Constitution. Stock. Bo Si s2 B, s4 s6 S6 s7 s8 No. Mean. a CdCd Misc S.Am .Do . . 2 1 6 6 6 4 7 2 3 1 11 10 17 4 41 36 15 85 81 2.45 1.40 2.06 1.00 5.51 4.33 3.47 4.20 4.73 0.99 .50 .94 1.00 1.05 .82 .88 1.02 1.32 Do r 2 1 1 . Do . Misc. . 2 7 9 18 16 9 4 11 2 37 24 6 9 17 3 19 17 8 23 1 3 Do JBW . Do fBW . 1 2 Do S.Am. . 7 17 13 2 4 7 3 . . Do . . I'M: 32 CdCa 9 (2) . . Do . . . . . Do . . . (1) 1 1 (1) 12 (1) (2) 3 (1) (3) (2) Cd: 8 CdCa iBW S.Am . . Do . . . 1 1 1 1 9 /l.oCdCd: 1.5 CdCa \CdCa by teal dCr: 20 CdCa . . Do . . . 10 (5) 6 (1) 9 (2) 4 (1) (2) 3 6 2 jCdCi by teat . . Do . . . . . Do . . . (2) 2 (2) |6CiCr: 11 GCa ..Do... 2 (2) 5 (1) 2 \ CrCr by teat . .Do... (1) 1 Totnl 2 7 6 10 7 2 3 3 2 1 1 21 17 4 177 81 42 258 303 98 401 1.95 2.06 1.00 4.47 4.73 1.90 4.55 3.95 4.45 4.07 .95 .94 1.00 1.13 1.32 1.06 1.20 1.53 1.54 1.53 36 16 5 52 66 18 84 54 24 1 78 70 26 96 48 17 2 65 58 20 78 31 17 5 4 3 CdiCdi 2 13 19 3 ::i 6 40 48 45 19 64 9 10 4 14 3 3 3 Dilute 2 2 20 20 Dil + RE Do INHERITANCE OF MINOR VARIATIONS IN INTENSITY. 91 yellow4 and cream7, mean at cream5.i very distinctly paler. Litters which should give both have given the entire range with two modes, at yellow3 and cream5, respectively. It is especially to be noted that among 13 of these, which were given grades before their constitution was known, 4 ranging from yellow2 to yellow4 proved to be homozygotes, while 9 ranging from cream5 to cream7 proved to be heterozygotes. Dilutes known to transmit red-eye (CdCr) have been either yellow4 or cream5, mean at yellow43. These should be compared with those of Table 39. — Effect of age on intensity of yellow. Constitu- tion. Stock. Mean. No. in sample. Mean at birth. Mean adult. Dark- ening. CdCd Do Do Do CdCr Misc-Dil Misc Dil j-i BW . S.Am . . . . . Do 2.8 5.5 5.5 4.8 4.6 4.3 9 17 9 11 9 5 3.1 5.1 5.2 5.0 4.4 4.8 2.9 5.0 6.0 4.7 4.7 4.2 0.2 .1 - .8 .3 - .3 .6 Table 40. — Effect of age on intensity of black. Constitu- tion. Stock. Mean. No. in sample. Mean at birth. Mean adult. Dark- ening. CdCd CdCa Misc.. . . . . Do 2.5 5.5 4.3 3.5 4.2 4.7 2.1 1.0 8 14 20 6 15 8 16 4 3.0 5.6 4.3 3.3 4.8 4.9 2.2 1.0 2.4 4.6 3.2 2.5 3.3 2.0 1.1 1.0 0.6 1.0 1.1 .8 1.5 2.9 1.1 0 Do Do . Do CdCr |BW.... fBW.... S.Am. .. Do ... . . .Do . . Do the same stock (S. Am.) which transmit albinism. The difference, yellow4.3 compared with yellow46, is too small to be relied on. Litters which should give both CdCr and CdCa have given a range of yellow4 to cream6, as expected. Thus grade yellow4 may be any sort of a dilute; one more intense is quite certain to be homozygous (CdCd) ; one more dilute is quite certain to transmit either red-eye or albinism. The influence of stock can only be recognized surely in the case of those known to be CdCa. The numbers are too small among the homozygotes. Among the heterozygotes (CdCa) it is clear that those of dilute and miscellaneous stocks, both with a mean of cream5.5, are distinctly paler than those with an admixture of BW or S.Am, blood with means from cream^ to creamy. The data in table 39 indicate that yellow undergoes no appreciable change in intensity during the life of an animal, except in the dilute selection stock. In this case there is a change from cream5.2 at birth to creamg.o when adult, among those carrying albinism. oj INHERITANCE in GUINEA-PIGS. VARIATIONS OF SEPIA. We find rather more overlapping of distributions among the sepias tli:m among the yellows when different genetic constitutions are com- pared. Nevertheless there are significant differences in the means. The groups ( !j< ,|. ( \\(\. and CrCr with means from sepiaj to sepia21, nearly black, average distinctly darker than groups CdCa and CrCa with mean- of >epia, - :m(l Bepia4.7, respectively. The case is quite different from yellow dilution in which CdCr and CdCa have the same effect (or nearly bo) contrasting with CdCd. Cr seems to be essentially identical with ( ',; in effect on black, but like Ca in effect on yellow. 1 ( n tint her analysis we must compare stocks. In the miscellaneous stock the average for CdCa is sepia5.5. When this stock is crossed with albinos of BW stock the average of the young— again CdCa — is sepia43. When these are crossed again with BW albinos the average becomes ia,5. The darkening influence of the BW stock is apparent. The South American stock also has a darkening influence with an average of sepia4 o. We find a similar difference between the miscellaneous and South American stocks among the homozygotes. The comparison of CdCa with CrCa within the same stock (South American) yields a slight but probably significant difference (CdCa, >epia, ,; CrCa, sepia47). Thus there is a difference of 0.5 with a prob- able error of 0.12. It is certain that some of the red-eyed sepias have been paler than any black-eyed sepia. If there is a real difference here, we wrould expect CrCr to be lighter than (\\(\\ or CdCn but the 4 individuals known to be CrCr give the darkest average of any array. They were not, however, a random sample and, further, were either pure lea or F2 IcaXBW and hardly t<> be compared in stock with those known to be CdCd or CdCr. For the present CdCl IMIKKITAM K IN < . I I NKA-l'K iS. independent factors p). I n t he albino series, the dilution and red-eye factors I | and C, respectively) produce nearly if not quite identical effects. In eye pigmentation, as in the black pigmentation of the fur, first-order effects may be due either to different combinations in the albino Beries or toother factors (p); but there is a sharp difference from th«' effects «'ii black fur, in thai the dilution and red-eye factors produce very different effects. In this ease the intensity and dilution factors apparently produce nearly identical effects. Table 41. \ bIIow fur. Black fur. Black eye. Formula. ( lolor. Formula. Color. Formula. Color. C CdCd CdCra ' !ra' 'ra ■ ■ Red Yellow. . . ( !ream . . White.... C- CdrCdr... CdrCft.... CaCa .... Black Dark sepia . Light sepia . White C- cd Cr CaCa .... Black. Nearly black. Red. Pink. 2. Second-order effects in dilution of yellow, black, and probably eye-color, are due to the unanalyzed residual heredity of different stocks. In the stock at the Bussey Institution BW and South American blood intensify as compared with dilute selection or 4-toe blood. This resid- ual heredity seems to be more important in the case of black than yellow, producing more overlapping of the ranges of the different albino series combinations. 3 . I n only one stock has the intensity of yellow at birth been observed to change appreciably in the lifetime of the animal. In this case, the dilute select ion stock, the creams grow paler as they grow older. Sepia, on the other hand, grows distinctly darker as the animals grow older in all -locks. In the imported South American stocks this darkening is 90 pronounced that adults of any albino series combination, except albinism itself (CaCa), are practically black. INHERITANCE OF VARIATIONS IN THE AGOUTI PATTERN. Most wild rodents and many other mammals have a coat color of the agouti type, viz, a predominantly black fur in which each hair has a subterminal yellow band. In many cases, as in the mouse and rat. the entire coat is fairly uniform in appearance. This is not true m .-ill cases, however. The color of Cavia cutleri has been described at the beginning of this paper. It will be recalled that the color of the belly i- Bharply distinct from that of the back, appearing wholly yellow instead of ticked. Tame guinea-pigs of the agouti variety likewise have this so-called light-bellied type of agouti. VARIATIONS IN AGOUTI PATTERN. 95 The agouti pattern of mice was shown by Cu6not in 1903 to be a unit Mendelian character dominant over its absence as found in blacks. In this and later papers (1903, 1904, 1907) he demonstrated that a white-bellied type of agouti and self yellow are due to members of the same series of allelomorphs. Castle, 1905, demonstrated that guinea-pig agouti is a simple dominant over non-agouti. This agouti pattern of guinea-pigs is subject to considerable varia- tion. In some cases the belly hairs are entirely yellow, a condition correlated with very broad yellow ticking in the dorsal fur. At the other extreme, the base of the hairs on the belly is black for about half the length, and the dorsal ticking is markedly decreased. This dark type has been produced by repeated crossing with intense blacks (BB race). Although distinctly darker than usual, all of the agoutis from such crosses are distinctly yellow-bellied. PREVIOUS WORK. Detlefsen (1914) made experiments with the wild species Cavia rufes- cens of Brazil. This has the agouti pattern, but is somewhat darker than C. cutleri or the tame guinea-pig. The yellow bands in the dorsal fur are narrower and there is usually more black on the belly, which indeed is usually slightly ticked with black. The difference in appear- ance is not very great. Detlefsen found, as he expected, that C. rufescens was homozygous for the agouti factor. In the hybrids between C. rufescens and black guinea-pigs, the agouti behaved as a simple Mendelian dominant. What was not expected was a marked darkening of the agoutis which occurred among the hybrids in many cases. The yellow subterminal bands became so reduced on the back that many of the agoutis appeared more like blacks than guinea-pig agoutis at birth. Black appeared at the ends of the hairs on the belly, and the appearance changed from yellow to ticked. In the early generations the variations in the agouti were exceedingly erratic in their hereditary behavior. Light-bellied hybrids crossed with blacks often gave ticked-bellied young, and ticked-bellied hybrids gave light- bellied young. Nevertheless, as more guinea-pig blood was introduced by repeated back-crosses, the trend was constantly toward the ticked- bellied type. In lines in which the ticked-bellied type had become constant, crosses were made with typical light-bellied agouti guinea- pigs. The ticked-bellied type was found to be recessive and segregated out in later crosses in regular fashion. Detlefsen found that the results in these lines were adequately explained by assuming that the ticked- bellied type is due to an allelomorph of both the light-bellied agouti factor and the non-agouti factor, recessive to the former, dominant to the latter. He used the nomenclature A, A', and a for the tame agouti, wild agouti, and non-agouti factors, respectively. 96 IMll i;ll ANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. TH£ INHERITANCE OF THE AGOUTI OF CAVIA RUFESCENS. The writer ha- had the opportunity of experimenting with the hybrid nil: stock developed by Dr. Detlefsen. As the mode of inherit- ; the fcype of agouti is of special interest in being a character in which two wild species differ, it seemed worth while to obtain additional data. New crosses were made to test out the hypothesis of triple allelomorphs as thoroughly as possible. It may be said at once that the result- obtained completely confirm Detlefsen's hypothesis. \\ hen received by the writer, there were only 2 light-bellied agouti hybrid- in the stock which had derived their agouti from C. rufescens. These were A606 and A450, \ and f blood hybrids, respectively. They were crossed with black guinea-pigs and one litter was obtained from li 2 bl.uks from A606 and 1 light-bellied agouti and 1 black from A.450. This light-bellied agouti son unfortunately proved to be sterile, so that experiments with light-bellied rufescens agouti came to an end. Only one light-bellied agouti — born dead — has appeared since then which nied to derive its agouti from C. rufescens, and in this case the parentage was doubtful. Thus in the following experiments, rufescens agouti and ticked-bellied agouti are practically equivalent. It must be emphasized that this was not the case in Detlefsen's experiments, that the following results are simpler than those which he encoun- tered in the earlier generations. Let us consider first the relations of rufescens agouti and guinea-pig non-agouti. Cross 1 gives matings of non-agoutis with ticked-bellies known to be heterozygous because of a non-agouti parent (table 42). Table 42. Female. Male. Agouti light- belly. Agouti ticked- belly. Non- agouti. la 16 le id U Non-agouti (g. p) Non-agouti (In brid) Non-agouti 'iti ticked-belly uti ticked-belly X agouti ticked-belly 1 62 17 11 61 10 62 13 12 63 5 X agouti ticked-belly X A'a (red or white) X non-agouti (g. p.) X non-agouti (hybrid) 1 161 155 The single agouti light-belly was the son of A450, mentioned above, which, though agouti light-belly, is included under agouti ticked-belly ae a rufescens agouti. The cross shows that ticked-belly is a simple dominant over non-agouti. The ratio of agouti ticked-belly to non- agouti is sufficiently close to a 1 to 1 ratio. If ticked-bellied agouti were due to independent modifying factors or to the residual heredity of rw, acting with the same agouti factor as found in C. cutleri and < . porceUus, non-agouti guinea-pigs should possess factors tending VARIATIONS IN AGOUTI PATTERN. 97 to change ticked-bellied agouti to the typical light-bellied type. The crosses show conclusively that they possess no such tendency. Indeed when it is recalled that, in the early hybrids and C. rufescens itself, light-belly was common, it seems necessary to suppose that guinea-pigs possess a residual heredity which tends to darken agouti. Ticked-bellied agoutis, known to be heterozygous because of parent- age, were crossed inter se. The results are given in cross 2. Agtb. X Agtb. Aglb. . 0 Agtb. 66 Non-ag. 19 This result is sufficiently close to the expected 3 to 1 ratio. One- third of the ticked-bellied young from this cross should be homozygous (A'A') and two-thirds heterozygous (A'a). Several of them have been tested by crosses with blacks (cross 3, table 43) . Table 43. Female. Male. Agouti light- belly. Agouti ticked- belly. Non- agouti. 3a 3c 36 3d 7 agouti ticked-belly Non-agouti 5 agouti ticked-belly Non-agouti X Non-agouti X 9 agouti ticked-belly X Non-agouti X 1 agouti ticked-belly 10 40 25 12 11 38 The single agouti light-belly was the one of doubtful parentage men- tioned above. Sixteen heterozygotes were obtained which gave agouti ticked-belly and non-agouti in approximately equal numbers ; 6 possible homozygotes were obtained, rather fewer than is to be expected. The male AA253 with 12 agouti ticked-belly young and 2 females, AA213 and AA217, with 8 agouti ticked-belly young each, were quite certainly homozygous and were used to establish a homozygous ticked-bellied stock. They and their progeny crossed inter se have given only ticked- bellies, 26 in number (cross 4). These homozygous ticked-bellies are indistinguishable from heterozygotes in appearance. Cross 6 gives matings of homozygous light-bellied agouti guinea-pigs with non-agouti hybrids. The young, 29 in number, are all light- bellied. There is no tendency toward ticked-belly introduced by th#» W{$S«f / givC. ™^ - ^gxx^mea nybrids (agouti derived from guinea-pigs) with non-agoutis. All of these light-bellies were known to be heterozygous from their parentage. The result, 18 light-bellies, 21 non-agoutis, no ticked-bellies, is in harmony with expectation (19.5 :19.5). , „ A. _ • ,. „ Crosses 8 and 9 give data on the relation of light-belly to ticked-belly. Homozygous light-bellied guinea-pig by ticked-bellied hybrid gives exclusively light-bellies, 50 in number. Light-belly is thus clearly ms INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. dominant. Beterxaygous light-belly (with a non-agouti parent) by heterozygous ticked-belly (also with a non-agouti parent) gave 16 light-bellied, 6 ticked-bellied, and 10 non-agouti young where expecta- tion is Hi : S 8. The results given bo far show that light-belly is dominant or at least epistatic over ticked-belly, that ticked-belly is a simple Mendelian dominant over non-agouti, and that the difference between rufescens :iiul poreeUiu agouti is not a question of residual heredity. The fact thai crossing with guinea-pig non-agouti increases the difference be- tween rufescens and porcellus agouti, instead of destroying it, shows t hat ruf( 8ft ns agouti does not contain the same agouti factor as is found in guinea-pig agoutis. Rufescens agouti must have an allelomorph of guinea-pig agouti, recessive to the latter. This leaves two possibilities. This allelomorph may be (I) the non-agouti factor or (II) a new allelo- morph recessive to the porcellus agouti factor, dominant to non-agouti (Detlef sen's hypothesis). Both of these explanations fit equally well all of the data given so far. Under (I) a guinea-pig light-belly is AAa'a', a non-agouti aaa'a',and a rufescens agouti aaA'A'. Under (II) these three varieties are AA, aa, and A'A', respectively. The critical u si is whether it is possible to produce light-bellies which are double heterozygotes AaAV, capable of having both ticked-bellied and non- mti young, as well as light-bellies when crossed with non-agoutis. 1 )ctlefsen obtained 5 light-bellied agoutis from the cross light-belly by heterozygous ticked-belly which bear on this point. Each of these had ticked-bellied young, but no non-agoutis. They, therefore, point toward hypothesis (II), which is also more probable a priori. They had, however, only from 3 to 6 young, 21 in all, so that it is not wholly certain that they would have had no non-agouti young if tested further. This point, therefore, seemed to the writer to be one on which additional data would be desirable, and special attention has been paid to it. The cross heterozygous ticked-belly by heterozygous light-bellies known from their parentage to be free from ticked-belly can be repre- Bented ;i- follows nnder the two hypotheses: Aglb. AgJb. Aglb. A '/lb. Auib. Nortrng. \ .' X AaaV = A;. V.-,' + Aaa'a' + aaAV -I- aaa'a'. (II) A'a XAa = AA' + Aa + A'a + aa -..r.'mti V.'wi-'.'.^v'0 expect 2 light-bellies to 1 ticked-belly to 1 non- '" ilv A; v ;1) ,,llls, ;,lso have the power of transmitting non-agouti. Under II such light-bellies (AA') should not transmit non-agouti. *f r 1 1 1 hall (.1 the light bellies should be of this type and the other hah should transmit oon-agouti but not ticked-belly (Aa). Thus, if a Lot:. Qumber of young ran be obtained from a light-belly from such acrofi J, which has had ticked-bellied young in crosses with non-agoutis, the presence or absence of non-agouti young is decisive between the VARIATIONS IN AGOUTI PATTERN. 99 two hypotheses. Cross 10 gives the results of the tests of light-bellied young from such a cross as described (table 44). Table 44. Females. Males. Aglb. Agtb. Non-ag. 10a 7 aglb . . . Non-ag . . 17 20 10c Non-ag . . 4 aglb .... 26 33 106 10 aglb... Non-ag . . IS 30 lOd Non-ag . . 5 aglb. . . . 45 50 In no case has the same animal had both ticked-bellied and non- agouti young. Some of those which have had ticked-bellied young have been quite thoroughly tested. Male M138 had 20 light-bellied and 19 ticked-bellied young. Male B121 had 13 light-bellied and 13 ticked-bellied young. Male M91 had 5 light-bellied and 11 ticked- bellied young. The chance that these can represent 2:1:1 ratios is negligible. Thus hypothesis (I) may be dismissed. Light-bellied agoutis demonstrated to carry ticked-belly have been crossed inter se (cross 11). They have given 25 light-bellies and 9 ticked-bellies, no non-agoutis. This agrees reasonably well with the expected 3 to 1 ratio. The remaining tables give the results of miscel- laneous crosses. All of them are in harmony with the hypothesis of triple allelomorphs. MINOR VARIATIONS. Thus there seems no doubt that the light-bellied agouti of Cavia porcellus, the ticked-bellied agouti of C. rufescens hybrids, and non- agouti form a series of triple allelomorphs. The question remains whether light-bellied Cavia rufescens hybrids possess a different allelo- morph from the ticked-bellied ones, or whether the difference lies simply in the residual heredity. There are no wholly satisfactory data bearing on this point. Nevertheless the fact that the darkening seems associated especially with certain stocks of guinea-pigs seems to favor the second view. The writer has crossed ticked-bellied agoutis re- peatedly with the intense blacks of BB or BW stock. Young have been obtained which were self black, except for a few ticked hairs in the chest and whiskers. One ignorant of their history would probably have classified several of them as blacks. Before they became adult, however, these black ticked-bellies acquired a uniform though very slight yellow ticking throughout the entire fur. On crossing such black ticked-bellies with a dull black stock (4-toe) there is a return to a more strongly developed agouti pattern. The young are uniformly ticked when born. Thus these variations in the agouti pattern seem related to the residual heredity of the stocks, possibly with the same residual heredity which determines the very intense development of pigment, I DO INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. especially black, in the BB and BW races, the feebler development in the 4-toe race, and the much feebler development in the wild species. If thifl 18 correct, the resemblance of light-bellied rufescens to light- bellied agoutis, like thai of the pale color of C. cutleri to dilute guinea- pigs, is secondary. In both eases the wild species possess a different allelomorph from the guinea-pig in the principal series of factors involved, but owing to different residual heredity, have a superficial smblance. THE INHERITANCE OF THE AGOUTI OF CAVIA CUTLERI. The writer has had the opportunity of working with the agouti of Cavia cutleri. Repeated crosses have been made with blacks of the BW race of guinea-pigs to see whether a ticked-bellied agouti could be obtained. While some ventral ticking has been observed in some cases, the J -blood cutleri hybrids are still on the whole good light-bellied lutifl. The cutleri agouti is unquestionably more resistant to dark- ening influences than was rufescens agouti. No results have been obtained yet which serve to differentiate it from the guinea-pig agouti. This is additional evidence that C. cutleri was ancestral to porcellus. The experimental results are given in crosses 68 to 78. Only one cross has been made between a cutleri and ticked-bellied rufescens hybrids. Male K56, a black | cutleri (f 4-toe), was crossed with two ticked-bellied agoutis, which of course had some rufescens ancestry. There were 6 young (3 blacks and 3 ticked-bellies) of which one was quite light and one was black, except for a few ticked hairs on the chest and whiskers. There was thus no very conspicuous tendency toward light-belly introduced bj' the cutleri hybrid. It seems safe to assume that C. cutleri has a different member of the agouti series of allelomorphs from C. rufescens, but the same or nearly the same as C. porcellus. Wild species of the same genus seldom differ as much superficially in any one character as do many varieties of domesticated animals. Yet while very large variations in the latter have been shown in many cases to behave as simple Mendelian units in inheritance, the char- acters by which wild species differ usually seem to be highly complex in heredity. Few well-defined Mendelian factors are recorded in the literature of hybridization. It is, therefore, interesting to find that the darker agouti of Cavia rufescens differs from the lighter agouti of C. cutleri by a clear-cut Mendelian factor. INHERITANCE OF ROUGH FUR. In the wild species of cavy, and in the ordinary smooth guinea-pigs, the hair shows a definite direction of growth, which is always away from the snout on the body and toward the toes on the legs. This is at least the general tendency of the hair in most mammals, and it is ROUGH FUR. 101 obviously the most advantageous; the hair lying thus is not ruffled or caught by obstacles when the animal is moving. This direction is not directly imposed on the hair by outside agencies, as might be supposed, but is due to the direction of growth of the hair follicles (Wilder, 1909). Certain fancy varieties among guinea-pigs, as the long-haired rough "Peruvians " and the short-haired rough " Abyssinians," show a striking deviation from the normal hair direction. In these varieties the coat can be divided into a number of areas, within each of which all hair directions radiate from a definite center. The boundaries of these areas, where contrary hair-currents meet, are marked by crests. The centers with their radiating hair-currents are called rosettes. Many mammals, including man, naturally show rosettes, crests, or other peculiarities of hair direction, but less conspicuously than the rough guinea-pigs. (See plate 7.) The positions in which rosettes may occur in guinea-pigs are quite definite. Following are the rosettes and irregularities given by Castle (1905), with the addition of L, irregular roughness on the chest. A. Forehead, unpaired. E. Sides, between shoulder and hip. I. Navel, unpaired. B. Eyes. F. Hips. J. Front toes. C. Ears. G. Above the groin. K. Hind toes. D. Shoulders. H. Mammae. L. Irregular roughness of chest. In the grading of the young guinea-pigs, large letters, as above, have been used for well-defined rosettes and small letters for feeble rosettes or slight deviations from normal hair direction in an area, indicated only by crests at a boundary. Thus a mid-dorsal crest or mane (e) without any side or hip rosettes is characteristic of a certain grade of partial roughs. The ear rosettes (C) are usually only revealed by a crest between the ears. The shoulder rosettes are seldom well developed. The side rosettes are sometimes doubled in the roughest animals (E,E2). The number of rosettes present varies from the full set described above, through a continuous series of intermediate grades, to one pair. The variations are not merely haphazard, but may easily be classified. In the first place, it is necessary to distinguish two series. A slight roughness found in certain stocks (the BW, lea, and Arequipa stocks) does not fit into the usual series of variations and will be discussed separately as series II. The roughness of the remaining stocks and also of the fanciers' "Peruvians" and "Abyssinians" we may call series I. In series I all the variations found may be arranged with con- siderable accuracy in a single linear series. Thus Castle (1905) used six grades, passing from rough A with the maximum number of rosettes to rough F, smooth except for the hind toes. These grades will be used in this discussion, with the exception that it has been found con- venient to combine grades C and D, leaving five grades, A to E. These letters used for grades must not be confused with those used to name the rosettes. 102 IMIKHITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. Reversal of hair direction on the hind toes is the most constant feature of the roughness of series I and has been found in all rough guinea-pigs of series I without exception. Following is the usual order of succession of the additional rosettes and irregularities found in pass- ing from a smooth to a full-rough: Hind toes K J il creel e Side rosttt<->, crest between eara E,C Forehead, hip. ventral rosettes. . . A,F,H,I,L Eye rosettes B Groin, shoulder, second side ro- settes G,D,lv There i- seldom more than a slight amount of asymmetry. As a rule the paired rosettes are present or absent as pairs. The most common exception is in the side rosettes in low-grade partial-roughs. Among these it is not uncommon to find a good rosette on one side and merely a slight change in hair direction on the other. In classifying, six of the most distinct sets of rosettes have been used as the principal criteria, viz, forehead, eyes, sides, hips, front toes, and hind toes. CLASSIFICATION. Hough A. — The forehead and five critical pairs of rosettes must be well developed. In addition, there is always some ventral roughness and a crest between the ears. Rough B. — This includes various conditions intermediate between rough A and rough C. Rough C. — There is only one pah' (or half pair) of well-developed r< iee1 1 < s. usually the side rosettes. There is always, in addition, rough- ness on at least the hind toes and usually a crest between the ears. Rough D. — A mid-dorsal crest is present and roughness of at least the hind toes, but no well-marked rosettes. Rough E. — Roughness is confined exclusively to the toes, usually to the hind toes. The following list shows the variations which have been met with in each grade; the letters represent the rosettes: Rouuli A. ABcEFHIJK to ABCDE1E2FGHIJKL. Rough B. \88. 53 69 Cross. 55 S.-i 38 \ Iri E Tri Bm l-toe E Tri A l toe, tri Sra pure lea. \ Sm pure rut Total Expectation Sa. Ss XSS C, D Tri E Tri C, D Tri Sm pure rut . Total B Espeetation Ss + SS. SS X SS E Tri E Tri Expectation SS B 2 13 5 3 23 D 30 D 11 E 15 11 B D E The interpretation given is no doubt open to objections. In some cases the ratios seem rather aberrant. This is in part due to the small numbers, but also to the overlapping of class ranges. In most cases rough B must be considered as full-rough genetically (Rss), but in some cases it is probably partial-rough (RSs). Rough E usually seems to be RSS, but in some cases must be heterozygous (RSs). It has not been demonstrated that factor S of the wild species is identical with the similar factor of the tricolor stock. If not identical, however, the latter stock differs from the wild by two mutations which neutralize each other, while if identical we can consider that the original tricolor stock had simply persisted in the primitive condition, never having had the rough intensifying mutation, s, of the fancier's roughs. MINOR VARIATIONS. Probably part of the minor variations in roughness are due to chance irregularities in development which are not hereditary. This is indi- cated by the Blight asymmetry not uncommonly present. This asym- metry seldom amounts to more than the absence of a member of one pair of rose! tee. No Mendehan analysis has yet been attempted for minor variations, but certain hereditary differences between different stocks are quite ROUGH FUR. 117 clear. The Lima stock shows a distinctly lower level of development of roughness than is found in the 4-toe stock or even among the full- roughs of tricolor stock. A large part of the variation and overlapping in the remaining experiments in which various stocks have been mixed is made intelligible by assuming that the residual heredity is unfavor- able for roughness in the wild species and especially favorable in the 4-toe stock. If we let 2+ stand for favorable and 2 — for unfavorable residual heredity, the wild species and presumably the primitive guinea-pigs are rrSSS — , while the good fancier's roughs, RRssS-f differ by at least three independent sets of factors, all favorable for roughness. ROUGHNESS OF SERIES II. It has been mentioned that irregularities in hair direction have been found in certain stocks which can not be classified by the grades which have been defined. The BW race is a highly inbred race. No indi- viduals of the pure stock have ever been observed to have roughness on the face, back, or toes, but many of them show irregular partings and crests along the chest and belly. It will be remembered that in series I ventral roughness appears only in high-grade roughs — grades A or B. Thus the characteristic roughness of the BW stock is nearly the least characteristic feature of series I. The only distinction which has been made in these BW roughs is between strong-rough with two or more ridges and poor-rough with only one ridge or a mere trace of roughness. Table 61 shows the principal results. Table 61. Smooth. Poor rough. Strong rough. Smooth X smooth Poor X poor Strong X strong 11 14 5 6 1 5 6 1 16 It is clear that this roughness is due neither to a simple dominant nor to a simple recessive. Aside from this, the results are exceedingly difficult to interpret, since poor X poor gives more smooth than does smooth by smooth. Probably the results will become more harmonious when more data are obtained. It seems safe to conclude at present that this roughness is wholly independent of ordinary roughness in its causation. Irregularity in hair direction on the back, not resembling anything in series I and not correlated with roughness of the hind toes, has been observed in a few individuals of Arequipa and lea stock. It does not seem to be like the BW roughness, but resembles the latter in the irregularity of its inheritance. US INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. SUMMARY. The principal results which have been reached may be summarized as follows: 1 . A clasMlieat ion of guinea-pig fur, skin, and eye colors is given with definitions of fur colors in terms of Ridgway's charts (1912). 2. Undent color factors are conveniently classified as follows: a. Factors which affect the distribution and intensity of color largely irrespective of the kind of color. b. Factors which govern the differentiation between yellow and dark colors in colored areas of the fur. r. Factors which determine the kind of dark color in the areas with dark pigmenta- tion in fur and eyes, without influence on yellow areas. Definitions of all known guinea-pig color factors are given on this basis and a table of the color varieties arising from combinations of these factors is given. 3. Genetic and biochemical evidence on the physiology of pigment formation suggests the hypothesis that the three groups of factors determine respectively the distribution and rate of production by the nucleus of the following substances: a. A peroxidase which, acting alone, oxidizes chromogen in the cytoplasm to a yellow pigment but is so unstable that it must be produced at a relatively high rate to give any pigment at all. b. A supplementary substance which, united with the first, makes it a dark-pig- ment-producing enzyme and of such stability that color develops at a much lower level of production of peroxidase than when the supplement is absent. Above the level at which both produce effects, the dark and yellow-producing enzymes compete in the oxidation of chromogen. c. Additions to the second substance which cause variations in dark color but not in yellow or in the competition between dark color and yellow. 4. There is a continuous series of variations in intensity of pigmen- tation in the yellow, brown, and black series and in eye color. The ordinary dilute guinea-pigs are found to be imperfect albinos in the sense that dilution is due primarily to a member of the series of allelo- morphs— intensity, dark-eyed dilution, red-eyed dilution, and albinism, with dominance in the order of increasing intensity. 5. A further step in the analysis of the continuous series of variations of intensity is taken in the demonstration that dilution is imperfectly dominant over red-eye and albinism as regards the yellow series of colors, and that dilution and red-eye are imperfectly dominant over albinism, as regards the black series. Smaller effects are due to the residual heredity of different stocks and to age. I ividence is presented which confirms the hypothesis of Detlefsen (1914) that the light-bellied agouti pattern of tame guinea-pigs, the ticked-bellied agouti of hybrids between the tame guinea-pig and Cavia rufcsccns, and non-agouti (as seen in self blacks or browns) form a Beriee of triple allelomorphs in which light-belly is the highest dominant and non-agouti the lowest recessive. Evidence is presented which GENERAL CONCLUSION. 119 indicates that Cavia cutleri possesses the same agouti factor as tame agouti guinea-pigs. Light agouti of Cavia cutleri and dark agouti of Cavia rufescens are thus variations in a character in two wild species which differ in heredity by a clear-cut Mendelian factor.1 7. There is a continuous series of variations between smooth fur and very rough or rosetted fur in guinea-pigs. The primary effects in this series are due to two independent pairs of allelomorphs. One factor, discovered by Castle (1905), is essential to any roughness of the common type, and is completely dominant over its allelomorph found in wild cavies and smooth guinea-pigs; the other, an incomplete recessive to its allelomorph in the wild cavies and some tame guinea-pigs, is necessary for the higher grades of roughness. Second-order effects seem to be due to the residual heredity of different stocks, and probably to non-hereditary irregularities in development. There is a roughness of a different type from the usual which is inherited independently. GENERAL CONCLUSION. Most of the successful earlier attempts at Mendelian analysis of heredity naturally dealt with variations which were obviously dis- continuous. But in nature such variations are much less common than apparently continuous series of variations. It was thus a common reproach against the Mendelian analysis that it dealt only with excep- tional conditions. The work of Nilsson-Ehle, East, and others has shown how quantitative variation may be brought under a Mendelian explanation. MacDowell (1914) presents data on size inheritance from this standpoint and discusses the literature up to that time. Recently two very interesting papers have been published (Dexter, 1914, Hoge, 1915) which analyze the heredity of certain very variable char- acters in Drosophila by means of linkage relations. Several of the studies in this paper deal with inheritance in continu- ous series of variations. The only general statement which can be made about the results is that there is no general rule for such cases. Intermediates between varieties which mendelize regularly have been found to follow very definite modes of inheritance, which, however, are very different in different cases and could not possibly be predicted a priori. On the other hand, each mode of inheritance is exactly paralleled by cases among the most diverse groups of animals and plants. It may be interesting to summarize the modes of inheritance of inter- mediates which have been found. An intermediate condition is sometimes found to be due to an inter- mediate variation of the essential hereditary factor involved, i.e., to an allelomorph. Thus yellows are intermediate between red and albino 1 It should be pointed out, however, that the original stock of Cavia rufescens used in these experi- ments included individuals of the light-agouti character as well as those classed as dark agouti. It seems quite likely that dark agouti arose as a recessive mutation in C. rufescens.— W. k. O. 12() INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. guinea-pigs in appearance, and we find an allelomorph intermediate in dominance between the intensity and albino factor to be responsible for their condition. Sepias are similarly intermediate between blacks and albinos and arc due to the same allelomorph of intensity and albin- ism. The Beries, light agouti of Cavia cutleri, dark agouti of C. rufes- ceru and black, furnishes another example due to triple allelomorphs. In other cases, the intermediate type is an unfixable one, due to imperfect dominance. Thus cream is the heterozygote between yellow and albino. A "razor back " rough (rough C or D) is the heterozygote between a type smooth except for the hind toes (rough E) and a full- rough (rough A). \ series of deviations from the original type may depend on the presence of a certain factor necessary for any deviation whose effect is modified to different extents by independently inherited factors. Rough A contains the same rough factor (R) as does rough E, but differs in possessing an independent factor variation (s) favorable for rough- ness. Most of the variation which we have ascribed to residual heredity probably comes under this head. Deviations from type, which apparently form a natural series, may be due to wholly independent factors whose effects are merely super- ficially similar. A pink-eyed pale sepia superficially seems as good an intermediate between an intense black and an albino as does a black- eyed sepia, yet the former is due to a variation which is wholly inde- pendent of albinism; the latter is due to an allelomorph of albinism. White-spotted animals are sometimes called partial albinos and con- sidered as natural intermediates between the self-colored type and albinos, but genetically they are wholly distinct. Black, agouti, and self yellow form a series which is due to three allelomorphs in mice, but in guinea-pigs two wholly independent sets of factors are involved. Final ly, we must recognize series of variations in which no Mendelian factors have yet been isolated. The series of white-spotted and yellow- spotted types and the series of polydactylous types are examples in guinea-pigs. Further, in all series of variations, to whatever extent analysis has been carried, there always remains some unanalyzed varia- tion. In many cases such variations are known to be hereditary and can be assigned to the residual heredity of particular stocks. Such unanalyzed variations, however, are probably in general complicated by variation which is not hereditary, due apparently to irregularities in development. If we can measure the importance of such non- hereditary variation by the extent of irregular asymmetry met with, it ery important in white and yellow spotting, in the variations in the development of extra toes on the hind feet, and is noticeable in varia- tion- in roughness. In the continuous series of variations several of these phenomena have generally been found together. In the series from smooth to full- TABLES. 121 rough we find a primary unit difference, a modifying factor, imperfect dominance in the effects of the latter, effects of residual heredity, and probably some non-heritable variation. In the series from red through yellow and cream to white we find multiple allelomorphs, imperfect dominance, and small effects due to residual heredity. In the series black through sepia to white, we find independent factors, multiple allelomorphs which show imperfect dominance, and rather prominent effects due to residual heredity and to age. This last series is interesting as at least a close parallel in appearance to the series of variations in human hair — black, brown, tow-color, to white. Thus in each case a complex of the most varied causes underlies an apparently simple continuous series of variations. EXPERIMENTAL DATA. EXPLANATION OF TABLES 62 TO 137. Crosses 1 to 15 include all matings recorded by the writer which involve the inheritance of agouti and in which at least one of the parents had Cavia rufescens ancestry. A large part of the remaining crosses are non-agouti by non-agouti, producing only non-agouti young. All the young in which the agouti factor should produce a recognizable effect, if present, are classified under the heads Lb, Tb, and Non, which mean light-bellied agouti, ticked-bellied agouti, and non-agouti, respectively. Most of these are the typical (black-red) light-bellied or ticked-bellied agouti or black. Those which are not typical, e. g., brown-red agouti light-belly, red-eyed sepia, etc., are described further under the column "Remarks." Those young in which the agouti factor can produce no visible effect, even though present (albinos, reds, yellows, and creams), are described under the column "Unclassified." Thus the exact color of every one of the young from each mating can be found from the tables, with the exception that white and red spotting are not noted. The matings in each cross are numbered in the first column. The number, description, and descent of the mother and father are given in the second and third columns, respectively. As in the case of the young, black-red agouti light-belly or ticked-belly or black, depending on the heading of the column, are understood where no description is given. The descent is indicated in most cases by a reference to the mating from which the animal was derived. Thus 36-4 means mating 4 of cross 36. In other cases the stock is indicated as BB or BW. The symbol ArF2 means F2 from crosses of Arequipa cf 1002 with guinea- pigs. In some cases merely the amount of Cavia rufescens blood is given. Thus M49, in the first cross given, was an ordinary ticked- bellied agouti from mating 9 of cross la. Referring to this mating, we see that his parents were female 84, a black of BB stock, and male All 21, a ticked-bellied agouti with TW Cavia rufescens blood. 122 INHKHITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. ( rossee 16 to 44 include all matings recorded by the writer in which there was dilution or red-eye in either parents or offspring, except for a few cases among the Cavia cutleri hybrids and cases of intense by dilute with only intense young. Some other crosses are included for special reasons bearing on the inheritance in the albino series. There is some repetition from matings outside of 16 to 44, but most of those outside are intense by intense, with only intense and, in some cases, albino v( Ring. As in the agouti crosses, all matings are numbered in column 1. The number, description, and descent of the mother and father are given in columns 2 and 3, respectively. All the offspring are classified under the heads Int, Dil, RE, or W, winch stand for intense, dilute, red-eye, and white (albino), respectively. A further description of all except the albinos is given under the column "Remarks." The attempt has been made to give the grade of dilution at birth for every dilute or red-eyed animal where known. Crosses 45 to 57 give the data on the inheritance of rough fur in the 4-toe and tricolor stocks. As before, the matings are numbered. The young are classified under the heads A, B, C, D, E, and Sm, which refer to the grades of roughness defined in the paper and to smooth. The parents and offspring were black (usually with red and white blotches) except for a few cases which are all noted. Such a symbol as red-B means a red of grade rough B. Crosses 58 to 62 give the results in the pure Lima stock and 63 the results in the cross of Lima with other stocks. Where no color is given black is always to be understood. Crosses 64 to 66 give the matings involving rough fur among Cavia ri'fi scens hybrids which were recorded by the writer. Cross 67 gives crosses of pure lea with rough A stock. Crosses 68 to 78 give all the data in matings involving Cavia cutleri ancestry made by the writer. The following symbols are used : AL'I.borAg = Black-red agouti, light-belly. AuTb = Black-red agouti,ticked-belly. B - Black. BrAgLb = Brown-red agouti, light-belly. BrAgTb = Brown-red agouti, ticked belly. Br = Brown. B = Red (black-eye). R(Br) = Red (brown-eye). 81 VgLb = Sepia-yellow agouti, light- bi-Uy. SI Vj'I'b = Sepia-yellow agouti, ticked- belly. = Sepia. BrYAgLb = Brown-yellow agouti, light- belly. BrYAgTb = Brown-yellow agouti, ticked- belly. LBr = Light brown. Y = Yellow (black-eye). Y(Br) = Yellow (brown-eye). Cr = Cream, used in compounds like Y. SAg(R) = Sepia- white agouti (red-eye). Sep(R) = Sepia (red-eye). W = White or albino. Red (p) = Red (pink-eye). Sep (p) = Sepia (pink-eye). In such •■xprossiona as S,Y,Ag the numerals stand for the grades denned in the text. In crosses 1 1 6, Lb and Tb are used at the heads of the columns to include any light-bellied or boked-belked agouti Non means non-agouti. V B, C, D. I ., .Hid Sm are used for grades of roughness and for smooth. TABLES. 123 Table 62. Cross 1. — Matings of non-agouti (aa) with ticked-bellied agouti (A'a). Each of the latter known to be heterozygous because of a non-agouti parent. Expectation: A'a X aa = A'a + aa (1 AgTb : 1 Non-Ag). la. Mother non-agouti, without rufescens ancestry. No. 9 Non-Ag. o^AgTb. Lb. TbNon Remarks. Unclas- sified. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 39 9 49 9 39 9 39 9 29 9 65 29 9 39 9 84 C22 C35 39 9 3W 3 W BW... 4-toe. . BW.. . 4-toe. . BW. . . BB. .. BW. . . BW... BB. .. Misc.. Misc.. 4-toe. . BW... Misc . . la-9. ld-16 , M49 ....Do... B5 ... .Do... B27 B30 B69 B191 A1121 A1474 jJg . . . . Do B171 la-4.. B117SCrAgTb ld-11 . Do ld-14 lo-l. la-1. la-5. A--- D44 Sep 16a-3 AA244 Sep 2-12 . BW43W BW.. Sep (R) S.Am ....Do ....Do D113BCrAgTb 3a-7 AA433a 36-4 6 W 1 W 5 W 1 W 2 W 2 W 4 W Total . 62 62 3SOAgTb, 2 Sep.... SCrAgTb, BrCrAgTb, 3 Sep. SCrAgTb SCrAgTb SCrAgTb, Sep 3 AgTb, SAgTb(R), 2 Sep(R). 1 W 5 W 1 W 2 W 1 W No. 10 11 lb. Mother non-agouti, with rufescens ancestry. 9 Non-Ag. A443 A1390 A1227 W /A1413 \A1291 W A1309 W A 1407 A1413 Ml 15 W /M114 \M90 Br M90 Br A1330 A 64 • • • • ii-... I 3Z. ... A — A-- 16-7.. le ' T2£ cfAgTb. A469 \ . . A1050 gSj. ^A781 if. )A1449 A- A1513 3^... A1449 ^ . . ....Do .... M189 lc-3. ....Do Do... A1331 rlg Total . Lb Tb 17 Non 13 Remarks. LBr SCrAgTb . Br AgTb . Br SCrAgTb, Sep. BrCrAgTb, Sep Unclassified. W R, R(Br), Cr(Br) Cr W 1L.M INHER] I \N< K l\ GUINEA-PIGS. T vhi.k 62 — Continued. lr. Male genetically, but not visibly ticked-bellied agouti. 1 8 i B 6 7 >* '.» . \..i,- U- cfAgTb. Lb Tb Nod Remarks. Unclas- sified. 131 \\ G. I-. i '.i < ; n A.412 R{ Do Br) ,l» . . 2 3 1 1 3 1 1 Se p G p Do 3 3 s< rAtf] 3 S( "r A Tb ;,s Bep I >il . 17 3l)('nB) Dil B42 W Lo-3 Do gTb Se SC p. . . 2 \Y W ■ Hi nil Do . 1 2 1 1 'rAcTb M -".'-' \|;(v, X 1 ) 1 8 W lc-5 Do SCrAgl n>, 2 Sep 4 \V M353 «3. Dn Se p Total 11 12 h/. Male non-agouti, without rufescens ancestry. No. 9A*Tb. d"Non-Ag. Lb Tb Non Remarks. Unclas- sified. 1 2 3 J 6 6 7 B 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 2] 24 a 06 \L'i.ii \ L66 4-to 2966 BB 3013 BB Do . e o 1 3 5 2 6 1 2 2 5 1 'A l.Mt Au'I.I) i '1 4 6 1 7 1 \;in ,'„ A3 1 1 ,'„ 1367 & Aim; j'g A.1068 it A 1171 3', A.1058, A 1 1 7 1 ^ Do Do . Do Do Do . 2996 BB 1357 BW Do . 3 5 2 1 2 2 3 1 1 9 1 3 5 2 2 2 W A 1117 .,K Do SCrAgTb. . 2996 BB 3013 BB 1357 BW . Do . Do 4 1 1 1 2 11 A 1450 3't All 17. Al 150 aV A15sl' iA A1583 ei4 A 1C77 a\ A.1678 8'4 B8 ld- B23 ld- B26 1-/ Ml 13 lb- w 2996 BB Do . Do . . -7 Do . Do . 2 7 1 1 2 -12 . . Do . 11 Do . -7 C20 Mis 86 W BW ; Mtl_' BzCrAgTb H^-10.... 2 Sop 1 '1 61 63 TABLES. 125 Table 62 — Continued. le. Male non-agouti (genetically) with rufescens ancestry. No. 9 AgTb. o71 Non-Ag. Lb Tb Non Remarks. Unclas- sified. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 A1146 -& A504 W & 2 2 Sep B132 ld-3 M293 Y 42-14 2 1 SCrAgTh B95 ld-4 Do B24 ld-4 Do 1 B52 la-3 M201 W 42-13 2 2 B33 1 transmit non-agouti, because of a non-agouti parent. Expectation: Aa X aa = Aa + aa (1 AgLb : Non-Ag). No. 10 11 la. Female AgLb. 9 AgLb. .V &601 AMI A963 A1310 A bill A1324 M102 Do 3CrAgLb 106-7. . D95S(rAnbb in', s. . D61 SCrAgLb .; 13-5. . . D63 SCrAgLb ^ 13-5... :,'■: 66-1 D69 SCrAgLb 18-5. M I. '5 SCrAgLb 13-7. Total cf Non-Ag. 103 224 A718 166 Do A719 W A462 W M2 20W toe. toe. ■1- 4- 4-toe . &■■ BW HW36WBW. ...Do Do Lb 14 Tb Non 18 Remarks. Sep SCrAgLb 3 SCrAg, 2 Sep 2 SAg(R), Sep, Sep(R) 2 SCrAg, Sep, . Unclas- sified. W 3 W 7 W W 76. Male AgLb. N... | 9 Non-Ag. 67 G.p . . D43 Sep 16a-3 M.'36Sep(R) ArF2. D45Sep 16a-3 cfAgLb. Lb Ml 23 13-2.. ' 2 D94 SCrAgLb 106-8 . 1 1 M331 BrCrAgLb 42-10. i. . . DS4 SCrAgLb 106-8.! 1 Total 4 Tb Non Remarks. SCrAg, Sep . SCrAg, Ag. Unclas- sified. W w 8i imauy of Cross 7. No. ' Aa (hybrid). aa Lb Tb Non la 71, 9 9 AgLb.... cfcf AgLb.... cfcf Non-Ag. . 9 9 Non-Ag. . 14 4 18 3 Total 18 21 TABLES. 131 Table 69. Cross 8. — Matings of ticked-bellied agouti (A'a) with homozygous light-bellied agouti (AA). Expectation: AA X A'a = AA' + Aa (all AgLb). 8a. Female AgLb. No. 9 AgLb. cfAgTb. Lb Tb Non Remarks. Unclas- sified. 1 2 3 4 5 03 G.p B5 ld-16 Do 5 4 4 8 4 3392 G.p SCrAg 02,03 G.p A1155 1*6 A1474 -L 20a G.p Do SCrAg Total 25 8b. Male AgLb. No. 9AgTb. cfAgLb. Lb Tb Non Remarks. Unclas- sified. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 B33 ld-18 B36 ld-18 2597 G.p Do 3 5 2 3 6 2 2 2 B37 ld-20 Do B52 la-3 Do /B40 W la-3 \B37, B33 above A702 ^ ) Do / Do A913 3^ Do 1AA606 SYAgTb 4-2 724 SAe(R} (lea) .... 2 SYAgLb . . Total 25 50 Total cross 8 !AA606 was A'A' Table 70. Cross 9. — Matings of light-bellied agouti (Aa) with ticked-bellied agouti (A'a), both known to be heterozygous with non-agouti. Expectation: Aa X A'a = AA' + Aa + A'a + aa (2 AgLb : 1 AgTb : 1 Non-Ag). No. 9 AgLb. cfAgTb. Lb Tb Non Remarks. Unclas- sified. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 3015 G.p M46BrCrAg 13-3 M57 13-1 A1474 TV A1170 & Do 6 2 2 5 3R W BrAgLb 1 1 1 1 1 A1310 ^ Do A1311 aSs Do A1449 3*2 Cr A1513 g^ A1449 aS, A1513 ^ A1HKO J_ 1 5 2 1 2 1 A 1 02fi k1™ Total 16 6 10 132 INHERITANCE IN CUINEA-PICS. Table 71. M /t>. — M:vt ihk- of tight-bellied agouti from such crosses as 8 and 9 (AA', Aa) with non- •uti. made in order to test whether light-bellied agouti can transmit both ticked- bellied agouti and non-agouti. I ipecUtion: AA' X aa = Aa + A'a (1 AgLb: 1 AgTb). or Aa X aa = Aa -f- aa (1 AgLb : 1 Non-Ag). 10a. Females Aa. So 9 A*] cf Non-Ag. Lb Tb Non Remarks. Unclas- sified. 1 a 3 4 5 0 7 B B120 85-2 B140 Bo-2 Do C20 G.p M328B-Y 42-17.... 20 W BW Ml 16 Sep 42-11 2 . . 3 .. 3 . . 3 1 2 2 M195 9-7 M217 8o-3 M282 15-12 A1562 W 3'f 2 Br 356 4-toe. . . . Ml 16 Sep 42-11.... AA83 Jj 1 . . 2 . . 2 2 2 Sep A1691 86-7 Do 6 .. 6 7 females 17 .. . 20 106. Females AA'. \" 9 AgLb. c? Non-Ag. Lb T b Non Remarks. Unclas- sified. l 2 3 4 5 fi 7 8 9 10 11 12 ASs'.) Br AgLb ,V . A499 BrAgLb fa . . . . A1688 86-6.. A511W fa AA83 Jj Do 1 . . 2 W,R R i : 2 : i i 3 : 4 I .... 1 .... BrAgLb.BrAgTb Br AgTb A1690 86-7.. Do 3R B139 8o-2.. Do B141 SCrAgLb 8a-2.. Do M328B-Y 42-17.... 20 W BW 1 .... 1 .... 1 .... 1 } 3 SYAgLb . . . SCrAgTb M328B-Y 42-17.... 20 W BW 356 4-toe 393 4-toe .... Ml 16 Sep 42-11 ... Do i : 2 2 . 2 2 1 SCrAgLb, 3 SY AgTb 2 SCrAgLb, SCr AgTb 6 W M25 9-1... M27a 9-1... M82 9-7. M92 Sa-4.. 5 .... i .... 1 SCrAgLb, BrY AgLb, SYAgTb 10 females 18 3 D TABLES. 133 Table 71 — Continued. 10c. Males Aa. No. 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 9 Non-Ag. 75 08 W 09 W M7 M7 AA58 M183 M261 Sep S7 S2 A1665 S7, A1665 B137 B133 B98 D14S W BB.. 4-toe . 4-toe . A-.- xV- &•• l 16 • ■ ■ 7a-7. 5S« • • 256 2B6 la-1. ldr-S. la-3. lc-8. cfAgLb. A581 Do ... .Do M133 16' J' Xa-4 . Do Do Do M205 Do Do Do B155 k...Do. 8a-4. 8a- 1 . D240 BCrAgLbl4-5. 5 males. Lb 26 Tl) Non 33 Remarks. BrAgLb SCrAgLb, Sep SCrAg, Sep Unclas- sified. 4R.Y? lOd. Males AA' No. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 9 Non-Ag. 31 M255 M253 M256 M254, M256 AA279 C22, AA278 B9 9 M168 M169 M171 M177 M86 M79W B31 B53 B54 C29 B9 9 M119 AA174 5 males. Misc la-10 la-10 la-10 la-10 3a-3 G.p., 3a-3 (above) . . A A lc-2 . . . 9-1 ... . A la-1 . . . la-3 . . . la-1 . . . G.p . . . (above) A 14-1 . . . d"AgLb. M138 9-1. . ....Do... ....Do.... ....Do... ...Do... ....Do.... ....Do .... ...Do... . 8a-4. M91 ....Do. ...Do M210 15-14. Do B121 85-2.. Do Do Do Do B130 8a-l. Do Lb 45 Tb 50 Non Remarks. SCrAgLb BrYAgLb, 2 BrCr AgTb SYAgTb BrAgLb . Unclas- sified. Summary of Cross 10. No. AgLb (Aa or AA') tested by cross with Non- Ag (aa). Lb Tb Non 10a 10c 10b lOd 17 26 20 33 Total 43 53 18 45 30 50 Total 63 80 134 IMIKIMTANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. Table 72. Cross 11.— Light-bellied agoutis (AA') crossed together. Botfa parents known to carry recessive ticked-belly by test (except in the case of AA533 with one young). peetation: AA' \ AA' = AA + 2AA' + A'A' (3 AgLb: 1 AgTb). 1 2 8 4 6 6 7 B 9A*Lb. o"AgLb. Lb Tb Non Remarks. Unclas- sified. 11890 s''-7 M25 9-1 M91 8a-4 Do 4 7 5 4 1 1 1 2 25 2 3 4SYAgLb M27a 9-1 . .Do M25 \! !7a 9-1 . . Do Hli'i Sa ' Do . M25 M27 B139 . ... . .Do 3 BYAgTb \ 1533 11-1 .Do \ \>n 11-2 Do 1 9 SYAgTb Total Table 73. Cross 12. — Miscellaneous matings of ticked-bellied agouti with ticked-bellied agouti. No. 1 o 3 4 5 6 7 9 10 11 12 9 AgTb. cfAgTb. Lb Tb Non Remarks. Unclas- sified. A A_Ui BrCrAgTb 2-5.... M19 2-16... AA16 A 3 3 1 2 3 3 4 / 1 4 2 1 1 SCrAgTb, LBr w w R w Do AA497 SYAgTb 10d-ll. \ A598 3c-22 A 1523 g'j M203 2-19... AA202 2-7 A A 206 2-9.... Do AA342 12-8... AA2S4 12-7 Do 1 2 LBr BrAgTb . . . SCrAgTb. . AA199 SCrAgTb 2-12 AA284 12-7 AA15 & Sep 2 SCrAgTb 4 SCrAgTb AA1 77 SCrAgTb lfr-3 AA507 3c-22 Do A1058 & AA242 SCrAgTb 12-8... M298 15-16 B 117 SCrAgTb ld-11 BrAgTb . . SCrAgTb . . Table 74. Cross 13. — Miscellaneous matings of light-bellied agouti guinea-pig with non-agouti hybrid. No. 9AgT.b. G.p 3444 G.p 01 G.p G.p 3266 BrYAgLb G.p 3220BrAgLb G.p 2718Ag(R) G.p 241 SA R AH 29 9G S R AH- I-', i 21-1 cTNon-Ag. >A1539 i.i A426R(Br) ,', . A678"W ,V Do M333 Y M34 Sep Mir.c, H M34 Sep .V • Kir l . 42 L7. liic 1 Lb Tb Non Remarks. SCrAgLb . 7 BrCrAgLb, 2 LBr 2 Br AgLb 4 SCrAgLb 2 Sep 3 SCrAgLb 2 SAg(R) SCrAg, Sep Unclas- sified. 3R 4 W TABLES. 135 Table 75. Cross 14- — Miscellaneous matings of light-bellied with ticked-bellied agouti. No. 9 AgLb. cfAgTb. Lb Tb Non Remarks. Unclas- sified. 1 2 3 4 5 f20a, 5a G.p. . . . \3015, 3014 G.p 20a, 5a, 3014 G.p AA173 14-1 . . . M82 9-7 4 9 9SAg(R) ArF2.. . }A412R(Br) & A1474 -^ AA199 SCrAgTb 2-12 . . . A1161 & 3 3 1 1 1 2 1 1 3R 4 SCrAgTb .... AA508 3d-2 . . . 13 9 SCrAg, BY AgTb Table 76. Cross 15. — Miscellaneous crosses involving the inheritance of agouti in rufescens hybrids. No. 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 9 Misc. AA171 R 14-1 . M137R 9-1.. Do M181 BrCrAgTb A1472 BrAgLb /A1562 W \A1688 AgLb /M27a AgLb \M 19 AgTb M 175 AgLb AA242 SCrAgTb /Ml 95 AgLb \M203 AgTb /B122 Agl.b \AA212BrAgTb /M92 AgLb \M106 Y? /M85 R \M82 AgLb / M56 AgLb \M50 AgTb AA28W 15-15 i 16 • • • JL 16 • • • Hb-6. 9-1.. 2-16. 8a-3. 12-8. 9-7.. 2-19. Hb-2. 2-6 . . 8a-4. 10c-2 lb-5. 9-7. 13-1. lb-7. A-- A ■ • lb-5 16 • • iV- 16 • • JL 16 • ■ 11-2 A1523 AgTb A1413B M84 R(Br) TA556 AgLb JA587W [A533 Y /A495 AgLb \A867 B /AA621 SYALb \AA621 SYALb .... 3 9 9 SAg(R) ArF2 198W ArF2 2 9 9 W ArF2 D125 W la-13 D427 W la-14 D86 W 76-3 . c^Misc. AA1161 AgTb^... AA286AgTb 3c-4. M29Br lb-6. C20B 163 B G.p.. 4-toe JL 64 l 16 AA83B 393 B A1040 B Ml 16 Sep 42-11 C20B G.p.. A1513 AA1 161 AgTb g>5. Al 170 AgTb ,&. Al 513 AgTb 3V M83 AgLb 9-7 Do A1161AgTb j^. JL 32 ■ Lb Tb Non •104 B 163 B 4-toe . 4-toe . BW36 W BW. 724SAg(R) lea. M224 BrAgLb 9-2 . M291 B M2B 133 SAg(R) 133 SAg(R) 3 32- A- 24- I20BWAg(R) 24-2. 3 21 1 2 1 Remarks. BrAgTb . BrAgLb . BrAgLb, SCrAg Tb, BrCrAg Tb SCrAgTb SSYAgTb 10 SAg(R), SAg Tb(R) SAg(R), Sep(R) SAgTb(R), Sep (R) 2SAg(R),3Sep (R) Unclas- sified. R,Cr 2R W R,2Cr W 3R W 3 W 136 INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. Table 77. Cross 16. — Matings of dilute with albino of intense stock. Expectation: CdCd X CaCa - CdCa (all Dil). CdCr X CaCa - CdCa + CrCa (1 DU : 1 RE). CdCa X CaCa = CdCa + CaCa (1 Dil : 1 W). 16o. Females CdCd. No. 9 Dil. o" W (intense stock). Int Dil RE W Remarks. 1 a 3 4 \ \fi21 SA'iAg 58 Si-Pi Do 39-4 Dil BW36 W BW 75 W BW 20 W BW... B42 W la-3 1 2 3 3 9 S6Cr6AgTb 2Sep3 2Sep3, Seps-Crj 3 S6Cr5AgTb Do Total 165. Females CdCa. No. 9 Dil. c?W (intense stock). Int Dil RE w Remarks. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 12a 13 14 15 16 15 Sep« 17 Cr, 30Cr6 55 Cr4(Br) /M42 LBr \M44Cr6 M42 LBr AA600 LBr-Crt M357 S4CrtAg B141 S4Y4Ag D43, D44 Sep3 D45 Sj-Cr6 D95 S3Y4Ag D95, M357 D67 Sj-Crj M384 Sep6 M442 BrCr6AgT 1 DG6 Sr-Cr6 Total Dil Dil Dil 75 W BW B42 W la-3 Do 4 1 3 2 1 4 3 6 4 1 2 2 2 1 1 32 2 Sep3. 2 Sep« Sep6 Dil Do 1 8 3 1 3 3 SBCr5AgTb 8Sep6 3Sep4 Sep4 S3Y4Ag, S6Cr6Ag Sep 4 SsCrsAgTb. 2 S3 Y4Ag Sepa-Crs Sep3, S3Cr6Ag Sep5, 2 Sep4 Sep4, Sep6 Sep2 42-12. . . 42-12... 42-12 39-19 42-1 39-23 16a-3 16a-3 166-9 . . . [l5 W BW Do Do 20 W BW.... Do Do Do 1 20 W BW.... . . Do . . 2 16c-4 39-12. . 3 39-12 406-13 Do 86 W BW Do 3 2 1 33 Do . . 16c. Males CdCa. No. 9W. c^Dil. Int Dil RE W Remarks. 1 2 3 4 5 6 132 W 12a W 5 W B2 W BWll \V (BWir. \Y 120. 21. J ;. 26 W Total ... 4-toe 4-toe BW A674 Sep« } 4 3 2 2 3 8 22 1 5 13 19 3 Sepj-Cr^, Sep 3 Sep8-Cr8 2 Sep6 S3-CrB, SsCr6Ag Tb 2 S5Cr6AgTb Sep< 2 S6Y«Ag. 3 S4- Y4, 2 S4, SB M34 Sep«-Cr4 16c-l Do BW BW BW 22- B117S«CriAgTb 39-14 l....Do / 13Cr,(Br) Dil TABLES. 137 Similar Matings from Crosses 19, 27, and 33, and Summary of Cross 16. No. Dilute. W (intense stock) . Int Dil RE W 16a 166 16c 5 9 9 Dil (CdCr) .... 3 c? & Dil (CdCr) W BW.... 9 3 5 4 9 33 22 10 5 "l 2 32 19 . ..Do 7 9 9 Dil (CdCa) .... 1 c? Dil (CdCa) .... 9 9 Dil (CdCd) .... 9 9 Dil (CdCa) .... d" o71 Dil (CdCa) . ..Do . ..Do W . . .Do . . Do . Total 85 15 60 Table 78. Cross 17. — Matings of intense from intense stock with albinos from dilute stock or from two dilute parents. Expectation: CC X CaCa = CCa (all Int). CCa X CaCa = CCa + CaCa (1 Int : 1 W). 17a. MaleCC. No. 9 W. cflnt. Int Dil RE W Remarks 1 2 3 M117 W 42-11 M327 W 42-17 3013 B BB 2 2 2 2B 2B 2B Do D 37 W Dil . . Do . . Total 6 176. Female CC. No. 9 Int. c?W. Int Dil RE W Remarks. 1 2 3 4 5 7 8 9 22, 23, 33 Ag Misc. 11 W Dil Do 11 6 4 4 4 2 2 1 11 Ag 6B Ag,3B 4 Br 4B 2B 2B AgTb C18 C50 Ag Misc.. (C24 Ag Misc |....Do . . Do \C34 B Misc 22 Br Misc . (S22B 5J« JM313W 42-16 . . Do 1A1665B 2ig 3223 B Misc. B232 B ld-21 M201 W 42-13 . . Do B23 AgTb ld-12 Total 34 17c. MaleCCa. No. 9W. 9 Int. Int Dil RE W Remarks. 1 2 D37 W Dil 06 B BW 2 1 2B B 9 W Dil Do 2 Total 3 2 138 INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. Taiile 78 — Continued. 17d. Female CCa. 9 Int. dHY. Int Dil RE W Remarks. 1 3 3d •l 5 0 7 B DlsAg 176-1 1 ) I'l \l' 1 T/i — 1 11 W Misc 2 2 1 2 1 Br. R Ag,2B 2 AgTb 2 AgTb AgTb, 2 B AgTb .Do .... 1 1 3 2 o ml i \„ 176-1 .Do 1 I >22fl \l' \7b-l .Do B33 A«Tb ld-18 B52 WTb la-3 M201 W 42-13 .Do HI 10 \cTb la-1 . Do . 1 1 Hill \t:Tb la-1 .Do 3 1 Hi's \<'Tb la-1 .Do Total 13 10 Summary of Cross 17. No. Intense (intense stock). White (dilute stock). Int Dil RE W 17a 176 17c 17,/ cTVIntCC.... 9 9lntCC... dVInt CCa. • W 6 34 3 13 2 10 Do Do 9 9 Int CCa • • Do Total 56 12 Table 79. ^ Cross 18. — Intense guinea-pigs, each of which had a dilute parent known to transmit albinism, mated with albinos or red-eyes to test whether the same intense animal can transmit both dilution and albinism. Expectation: CCd X CraCra = CCra + CdCra (1 Int : 1 Dil). CCa X CraCra = CCra + CaCra (1 Int : 1 RE or W). 18a. Male CCd by test. No. 9 Red-eye. d"Int. Int Dil RE W 3 1 3 3 3 4 3 3 8 3 3 2 3 1 2 1 7 2 25 30 Remarks. 515SAg(R) ArF,. 774SAg(R) ArF,.. 515, 774SAg(R) ArF2. D 10 R (Br) 35-1 . . Do Do 10 •".11 Sep (R) 281 SAg (R) 7 1 1 SAg (R^ •_• il BAg i: 113 - 7.V..SA ■ i: ArF2. ArF2. ArF2. \.l \H ArF,. Do D 30 R (Br) 36-1 . . . Do AA50S AgTb :W-2.. Do M4308Ag(R) 1-. i Total, •* malt Do Do 3Ag,SCrAg,2S3Y4Ag Ag 3 Ag,2 S2Y4Ag, S4Cr6 Ag 4 B.2 S2-Cr6, S4-Y4 3 Ag,S3Y4Ag, S4Y4Ag S6Cr6Ag, 2 S.Y^g 2 S4Cr5Ag, S6Cr6 Ag 3 Ag,2 S3Y4Ag, S6Y4 Ag 2 B,S3Cr6Ag 3 Ag,BiY4AgTb,SsCr6 Ag Ag.SaY^g, S,Y4Ag 4 S4CrsAg, S6Cr6 Ag 2B TABLES. 139 Table 79 — Continued. 186. Female CCd by test. No. 9 Int. cf Red-eye or \V. Int Dil RE W Remarks. 1 2 3 M292 B ii . . . . M353 B ei- ■ ■■ D 18 W 166-3 . . Do 2 1 2 5 3 1 1 5 AgTb, B,S6Cr6AgTb, 2Sep6 AgTb.SepB (Cross 20) D6a R (Br) 35-1 . . Total, 3 females. . . 724 SAg (R) lea 18c. Male CCa by test. No. 9 Red-eye or W. dTnt. Int Dil RE W Remarks. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 271 SAg (R) ArF2.. 278 SAg (R) ArF2.. M224 BrAg 406-12 . Do 5 2 4 5 3 5 3 2 1 3 1 1 1 2 3 5Ag,2S4Ag(R),S7Ag (R) 2 Ag,5SAg(R) 4Ag,SsAg(R),SAg(R) S4AgTb (R) 2 Ag, 3 B,2 SAg (R) AgTb.SsAgTb (R) Sep8 (R) 2 AgTb,Sep4 (R) 4B,Sep6(R),2Sep(R) 2 Ag,SePl (R) Ag, B 2 Br, Ag 5 B, Ag 11 B 2 Ag, 8 B 716 SAg (R) ArF2 . .Do 233 SAg (R) ArF2.. 773 SAg (R) ArF2 . M156 R (B) TV ... Do 236 Sep (R) ArF2.. 264 Sep (R) ArF2. . AA433a AgTb 36-4 . . . Do 1 2 4 2 2 2 1 3 1 485 Sep (R) ArF2.. 235 Sep (R) ArF2.. 693 W ArF2 . . D7 R (Br) 35-1 . . . D13 R (Br) 35-1 . . . Do D42 W 166-1 . AA578 W 3c-18 . 3 9 9 W ArF2.. 4 99 W Misc. . 3 9 9 W ArF2.. Total, 8 males. . Do Do 3 6 11 10 57 M291 B & M339 B 40a-14 . M2B & 20 9 13 7 41 1 10 INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. Table 80. Cross {£.— Matioga <>f dilute from cross 18a or 43, with albino. Expectation: C& X CaCa = CdCa + CrCa (1 Dil: 1 RE) (1-6). CdCa X CaCa = CdCa + CaCa (1 Dil : 1 W) (7-13). 10 11 12 13 14 9 Dil lor \\ nT-1 BtY«Ag L8a 5 D63S4Y4A4 13-2.. D121 W L8e 9. [67 W HI is w I \Y D61 S»Y«A« D241 BeOiAg D69 S4Cr>Ag M425 SYAg B772 W 266 W S781 W D69 S4Cr6Ag D20G Sr-Yi 22 ■'• L« 8. . 18c-6. 43-2 . . ls(1 '.1 18a-5. 43-1 . . ArF2.. ArF2.. ArFi.. 18a-5. 18o-4. d*W (or Dil). BW36W BW Do D71 SjCrsAg lsa-5. Do D240 S2Y4AK 18a-9 . Do BW:iii w BW. BW50W BW. BW36 W BW. D70S4Y4Ag 18a-5. Do Do BW36W BW.. BW50W BW.. 2 females CdCr . 2 males CdCr. . 3 females CdCa . 1 male CdCa . . . 2 females (?) . . . Int I)i RE W Remarks. S4-Y4,S3Ag(R),Sep,(R) Sep4, 2S3Ag(R),Sep4(R) SjCrjAg, S4-Cr6, S6Ag(R) Sep, (R) SsAgtR), Sep8(R) S4Cr6Ag, Sep4 2 SBCr6Ag, S6-Cr6. S4Ag (R) SeY.Ag 2 S4CrBAg, Sep4 S4Cr6Ag 3 S4Cr5Ag SsY4Ag, S4-Cr6 3 S.Y.Ag Sepfi Table 81. Cross 20. — Matings of pure lea male 724 CrCr. Kxpectation: CC X CrCr = CQ. (all Int) (1). CCd X CrCr = CQ + CdCr (1 Int : 1 Dil) (2-3). CdCd X CrCr = CdCr (all Dil) (4-5). CdCa X CrCV - CdCr + CrCa (1 Dil : 1 RE) (6). No. 9 Misc. cf Red-eye lea. Int Dil RE W Remarks. 1 2 3 4 r, 6 5 9 9 B Trior4-toe D6a R(Br) 35-1 724SAg(R)Ica... Do 9 2 1 9Ag 2 Ag, S2Y4Ag Ag 2S2Y4Ag 3 SaY4Ag B^r^Ag, SeAg(R) 1 D209R(Br) 36-3 Do . AA6068jY»AgTb 40a-8. . . .Do . 2 3 1 1 AA62 1 39-4 Do SA61 Sep4 32-2 Do . . Table 82. Cross 21. — Matings of pure lea male 575 CCr. 1 xpectation: CCr X CaCa - CCa + CrCa (1 Int : 1 RE). No. 9U. eflnt lea. Int Dil RE w Remarks. l 5 9 9 W BW . . . 575 An Ioa . . 9 4 4 Ag, 5 B, 2 S«Ag(R), 2 Sep4(R) TABLES. 141 Table 83. Cross 22. — Matings of intense Fi lea (cross 21) with albinos. Expectation: CCa X CaCa = CCa + CaCa (1 Int : 1 W). No. 9 W (or Int. Fi lea. cflnt Fi lea (or W). Int Dil RE W Remarks. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 2 9 9 W BW 141 W ArF2.... 13 Ag 21-1 . . . Do 4 3 4 4 2 2 4 6 4B 2B 2B Ag, 3B 4B M79 W g1* D17W 166-3... 14 B 21-1 . . . Do 2 19 Ag 21-1 .... IllAg 21-1 cT W BW.... Do 2 4 4 17 B 21-1.... Do Total 16 25 Table 84. Cross 23.— Matings of red-eye Fi lea (cross 21) with albinos of intense stock. Expectation: CrCa X CaCa = CrCa + CaCa (1 RE : 1 W). No. 9 White. d* Red-eye Fx lea. Int Dil RE W Remarks. 1 8W BW.... I5Sep4(R) 21-1 3 3 Sep3(R) Table 85. Cross 24- — F2 from red-eye Fi lea (cross 21). Expectation: CrCa X CrCa = CrCr + 2 CrCa + CaCa (3 RE : 1 W). No. 9 Red-eye Fi lea. d"Red-eye Fi lea. Int Dil RE W Remarks. 1 2 12 S4Ag(R) 21-1 . . 16 S4Ag(R) 21-1 15 Sep4(R) 21-1 . . 5 12 5 1 (S2,S3,S6)Ag(R),S4(R),S5 (R) (B0,3S2,S3,S6)Ag(R);(B0l S2, 2 04, S5, 06) (R) Do Table 86. Cross 26. — Matings of F2 lea (cross 24) with albinos. Expectation: CrCr X CaCa = CrCa (all RE) (1-7). CrCa X CaCa = CrCa + CaCa (1 RE : 1 W) (8-9). No. 1 2 3 3a 4 5 6 7 8 9 9 White. c? Red-eye F2 lea. Int Dil RE W Remarks. D86W 43-3.... M431 W 18c-4. . 126 BoWAg(R) 24-2 . . 5 6 5 4 5 2 1 2 3 2 1 S4Ag(R),S6Ag(R),S3 (R),2S6(R) 3S4Ag(R),2S6Ag(R), Sep4(R) 2 S5(R), 3 S«(R) S3, S4, Ss, S6 3 S6(R), 2 S6(R) S«Ag(R), S6(R) S6(R) S6AgTb(R),S6(R) 3 S4(R) Do D76, D78 W 18c-14 . . D76 W 18c-14 D77 W 18c-14. . 137 B0(R) 24-2 . . . . Do ... Do D125W 16c-^... BW48 W BW . 133 S2Ag(R) 24-1 . . . . Do D427 W 44-3 . .Do D73 W 42-6 S755 W ArF9 134 Sep4(R) 24-1 . . . Do L42 INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. Table 87. Cross 26. — Matings of red-eye F, lea (cross 21) with dilute. Expectation: CdCd X CrCa = CdCr + CdCa (aU Dil). CdCtt X CrCa = CdCr X CdCa + CrCa + CaCa (2 Dil : 1 RE : 1 W). No. l 4 9 Dil (or Red-eye l'i Eos. cTRed-eyeFiIca (or Dil). Int Dil RE W Remarks. \ \ju-A A/H. 40a-6.. /AA244Sep4 39-15.. \M261 S(-p4 41-2... I2S«Ag(R) 21-1... I6S1WR) 21-1 15 Sep4(R) 21-1 . . . \ ... Do 3 8 1 2 4 5 BiCr.AgTb, Bi- Crs, Sept f5 Bi. S«, S6-Cr6, S8, S,(R),2S4(R), I S,(R) S3 S7Cr5Ag, Sr-Crj M34 SeP6-Cr6 16c-l . . . . Do Table 88. Cross 27. — Matings of dilute from cross 26 with albinos. Expectation: CdCr X CaCa = CdCa + CrCa (1 Dil : 1 RE) (1-6). CdCa X CaCa = CdCa + CaCa (1 Dil : 1 W) (8-12). N... 9W (or Dil). cfDil (or W). Int Dil RE W Remarks. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 3 9 9 W Misc.. . D221 W ,«t G30 W St ... . DS9 Sep3 26-3 D196Bi 26-2 Do 4 1 1 6 3 3 2 1 3 2 2 1 2 S«. S8-Y4. Ss-Crs, 2 S,(R), Ss(R), 2 Sr(R). Ss(R) s, S5, S4(R), Ss(R), S7 (R) 3 S,(R) 3 S4, 2 S8(R) 54, S6-Cr6AgTb 55, 2 S5— \ 4 S5, Sc, S4, Se— Cr« 2 S4, 2 S4-Y4 S4 D115Bt-Crf 26-1... D197B, 26-2... D19SB, 26-2... BW4:; \\ BW.... 482 W ArF2... D42 W 166-1 . . BW56. 57 BW.. D1223rCrsAg 26-4... Dl95Sep4 26-2... BW50 BW . . \bW46 BW D113BiCr6AgTb 26-1 D123 Si-Cr5 26-4 D55 S5-Cr5 20-9, D114Sep3 26-1 BW50 BW . . 3 2 3 4 4 Do 1 Table 89. Cross 28. — Matings of pure Arequipa male 1007 CdCd with black guinea-pigs. Expectation: CC x CdCd = CCd (all Int). CCa X CdCd = CCd + CdCa (1 Int : 1 Dil). No. 1 3 9 In' 29 9B Woe 4 9 9 B BW 1442 B BW o" Dilute (Arequipa). l1007SYAg .... Do . Do . . . Int Dil RE W 4 10 3 2 Remarks. 3Ag,B 5 Ag, 5 B 2 Ag, B, SCrAg, Sep3 '1007 8YAg< dCdfron 1001 BRAgCCdand 1002 SCrAg CdCr pure Arequipa stock. TABLES. Table 90. Cross 29.— Matings of intense Fi (cross 28) with d"1007. Expectation: CCd X CdCd = CCd + CdCd (1 Int : 1 Dil). 143 No. 9 Intense ArFi. d* Dilute ( Arequipa) . Int Dil RE W Remarks. 1 2 3 SA4 Ag 28-3 SA8 Ag 28-2 4007 SYAg Do 3 2 3 2Ag,R Ag, R, Bi-Y3( Y3 2 Ag. B, BiYsAg, Y, 2 2 /SA4, SA8 l....Do \SA10 B 28-2 / !1007 SYAg CdCd from 1001 BRAg CCd and 1002 SCrAg CdCr pure Arequipa stock. Table 91. Cross 80.— Mating of dilute Fi (cross 28) with . station: CdCa X CaCa = CdCa+CaCa(l Dil: 1 W). l 9 Dil ArF,. c? White. Int Dil RE W Remarks. HAS Smh 28-3 75 W BW 1 Table 95. Cross 34. — Matings of intense Fi Arequipa with albinos. Expectation: CCd X CaCa = CCa + CdCa (1 Int : 1 Dil). No. 1 2 3 4 5 6 9 Int ArF! (or W). cfW(orlntArFi). Int Dil RE W Remarks. SA4Ag 28-3.... SA10, 11, 13 B 28-2,3. . M313 W 42-16 Do 4 3 3 1 2 2 8 2 1 1 6 2Y4 3 B, R, 2 S3, 4 S3-O6. 2 Y« 2 Ag, B, S3Y4Ag, S3Cr6Ag 2 Ag, B, S* Ag, S3 Ag,B,S3Cr6Ag,2S6Y4Ag, 2 S6Cr5Ag, Sj-Crs 149 VI 22-2 161 W 24-1. . . . SA26 Ag28-1.. . Do 349 W ArF2 Do 149, 349 W Do Total . 13 20 Table 96. Cross 35. — Mating of cream of dilute selection stock with a red stock free from albinism or dilution. Expectation: CC X CdCa = CCd + CCa (aU Int). No. 9 Intense. d1 Dilute. Int DU- RE W Remarks. 1 4 9 9R(Bi)Misc 00 Cr6(Br) Dil 12 11 R(Br), Y2(Br) Table 97. Cross 36. — Fi (cross 35) mated with father. Expectation: CCd X CdCa = CCd + CCa + CdCd + CdCa (2 Int : 2 Dil) (3). CCa X CdCa = CCd + CCa + CdCa + CaCa (2 Int : 1 Dil : 1 W) (1-2). No. 1 a a 9 Intense Fi. D261 Cr6(Br) Dil 10 7 L46 INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. Table 100. —All matmgH of intense with intense which have given dilute young, except thcwe given in cross 31. ftmeeUtion: CCd X CCd = CC + 2 CCd + CdCd (3 Int: 1 Dil) (1-7). CCd X CCft = CC + CCd + CCa + CdCa (3 Int : 1 Dil) (8-33). 3, 9, 11, 24, 26, 30, and 32 not wholly certain. No l a 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 _'l 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 9 Int. M35 ATM i AgTb B68 AnTI> M25 AgLb 3 A ld-15. 9-1. AA>s An 39-4.. M25,M27aAg9-l. . . \B139Ag 39-23. M177B lc-2.. M168B fV--- M169, M171B &..., B68 AgTb A443 B M90 Br M90 Br M114B A1117B A1566 AgTb la-1 16-7 Do. A529 BrAgTb ft. \A_'02AgTb 406-8. M177B lc-2.. Ml 02 AgLb 3392 AgLb 3392, 3444 Ag 3392 Ag A1310Ag M203 AgTb M82 A« AA171 H Ml S3 B 20 B M7 B Al 120 !'. ". B 66-1 . Misc Misc Misc Misc *v 2-19. 9-7. . A iV;.. Mi.-i- rV it- d" Intense. SALT, Ag 28-1, A 78 1 AgTb &.. B 118 AgTb ld-6. M91AgLb 8-4.. .... Do > Do Do Do Do B 118 AgTb ld-6.. A469 AgTb \ M 189 AgTb 39-30. Do 1357 B BAY . A 1050 AgTb 3V . . A A 15 AgTb Jj. .... Do .... Do AA235 AgTb 406-7 . M2B A1539B .... Do ... . B5 AgTb A 1474 AgTb A1449 AgTb AA284 AgTb A 1161 AgTb Do AA299 AgTb A412 R(Br) M133 Ag A811 Br 12845 B lrf-16. 39-18. 40-6. 8-4. . 4- toe. Int 109 Dil l47 RE \Y Remarks. Ag, B, R, Si-Yj 4 AgTb, 3 R, BrYAg Tb, Y 5 AgTb, 3 BrAgTb, B, SsYaAgTh, S, 3 Ag, 3 AgTb, 2 SYAg Lb, 2 S,Y2AgLb 2 Ag, S3Y^gTb Ag, 2 AgTb, B,YiAg Tb Ag, 3 AgTb, SYAgTb 2 Ag, 2 AgTb, SCr6Ag 3 AgTb, 2 BrCrAgTb, BrYAg 4 AgTb. 2 B, SjCrs AgTb LBr BrCrsAgTb, S5, Crj 3 AgTb, 3 B, S5Cr5 AgTb, S6 AgTb, B, S4Cr6AgTb 3 AgTb, BrAgTb, SCr AgTb, S4 AgTb, B, SCrAgTb 3 AgTb, B, BrAgTb, SCrAgTb, BrCrAg Tb 2 AgTb, 2 SCrAgTb AgTb, 2 B, BrAgTb, LBr-Cr5 2 Ag, 2 B, S6Cr5Ag SCrAg 2 Ag, B, SCrAg 3 Ag, S4Y4AB 3 Ag, SCrAg AgTb, B, Cr 3 AgTb, B, S AgTb, SCrAgTb AgTb, R, Cr 3 AgTb, SCrAgTb 2 AgTb, B, SCrAgTb 2 Ag, 2 B, SCrAg. S« Br, LBr 4B, S ISxeeM of dilutes expected because the presence of at least one dilute young is used as <» criterion f' \irTb 41-4 AA177S6Cr6AgTb 41-4.. .Do AA671 AgTb 40a-7.. Mil! BrC'r«Ag 44-1... Sep ArF2... Total AA253 S6Cr,AgTb 406-8. Al 170 AgTb sV-..- M156R iV 2 4 24 1 152 •Expectation: CCa X CdCa = CCd + CCa + CdCa+ CaCa (2 int : 1 dil : 1W). 'Excess of dilutes and albinos expected. Table 102. Cross 41. — All mating8 of intense with albino which have given dilute young, except those given in crosses 18 and 34. Expectation: CCd X CaCa = CCa + CdCa (1 Int : 1 Dil). No. 9 Int (or \Y). c?W (or Int). Int Dil RE W Remarks. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 All u. AgTb ,V . . . M102 AgLb 8&-1. . B13' 39-23. A1227W «V • . ■ Al.JD'.tW j'j.... LA28W J*.... A504 W ,'fl A462 W ,'« . . . 20 w b\y . A781 AgTb £t \l.--i:> AuTb .,'... . Do 1 1 2 4 4 1 13 2 1 1 1 1 4 1 ll1 S, S-Y T> Q AgTb, S3Cr8AgTb 2 AgTb, S6Cr6 AgTb BrAgTb, B. R, R(Br), Cr(Br) Ag, BrAg.AgTb. R, SCrAgTb, BrCr6AgTb, 2 Cr B, S-Cr 131 W 4- Total A412R(Br) fc. • ■ 'Excess "f dilutes expected though not found. TABLES. 149 Table 103. Cross 42. — All matings of dilute with dilute, except those of crosses 30 and 37. Expectation: CdCd X CdCa = CdCd + CdCa (all Dil) (M328, AA242, M394, CdCd). CdCa X CdCa - CdCd + 2 CdCa + CaCa (3 Dil : 1 W). No, 3 4 5 5a 6 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 9 Dilute. B141 S4Y4Ag 39-23. AA242 S3Y3AgTb 40a-6. AA244 Sep4 39-15 . D44 Sep3 16a-3 . D43 Sep3 16a-3 . D45 Sep3-Cr6 16a-3 . D26 S5Cr5AgTb 16a-4 . /DllOSepB 16c-3. \D107 Sep4 166-1 . D215Sep3 16a-2. M181 BrCr6AgTb 41-6. . 3520 Y4(Br) Dil . . . 3417 Cr6 Dil . . /3417 Cr6 Dil . . \3462 Cr6(Br) Dil . . 06 LBr-Cr Misc. M127Cr5 42-13. M44Cr5 42-12. M164Cr7 44-6.. M126Cr6(Br) 42-13. M164Cr7 44-6.. M44 Cr6 42-12 . M296 SCrAgTb 39-27. M310 Sep7-Cr7 40a-16. M336Sep6 40a-17. M336 Sep6 M394Sep4 42-22.. /M336, Sep6 40a-17 . \M394Sep4 42-22.. M393 Sep5 42-22 . . D278 Cr6(Br) 386-5 . . Bep i'e • • • ■ 3 9 9 W Misc.. B176B-CI ArF*.. 8263 S( ThXe. ArF2. A678 W A A868 W jV 9 1 5 2 1 2 1 3 7 SCr&Ag, 2 Br-Cr« S S4, S5Cr6AgTb, Br Cr«AgTh. 2 S, 2 Ss-Y* S4Cr6Ag S7Cr7Ag, Cr7 B117S4Cr6AKTl) 39-14 11 W Dil ... . . . Do . . 07 W Misc.. A674 Sep« J Table 106. Cross 45. — Rough A (4-toe) X rough A (4-toe). Rrss X Rrss = 3 Rss + rrss (3 A : 1 Sm). No. 9 Rough A. c? Rough A. A B C D E Sm 1 2 3 4 5 3769 4-toe 96 4-toe 3609 4-toe Do 1 2 1 /3769 4-toe \3770 4-toe 3769 4-toe 3770 4-toe | .... Do 3987 4-toe Do . . 5 2 2 Total 10 3 Table 107. Cross 46. — Rough A (tricolor) X rough A (tricolor) ; one or both of parents of each, rough C or D. Rrss X Rrss = 3 Rss + rrss (3 A : 1 Sm) (1-8). or Rrss X RRss = Rss (all A) (9-12?). No. 9 Rough A. cf Rough A. A B C D E Sm 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 Id 11 12 4018 Tri 3941 Tri 3943 Tri 3775 Tri 1 3940 Tri Do 3 1 7 2 2 1 1 2 3 2 3 fR66 54-17 3943 Tri R65 54-17 }....Do Do 1 2 2 RlTi 47 :'..... Do R278 52 1 1 R357 Bed ">'_' 8 R248 52-10 Do 1 1 1 R65 54-17 R171 47-3 R197 52-14 Do Rl'U 64-1 Do EU96 52 ll Do Total 1 to s 17 10 2 <7 4 Total 9 to 12 TABLES. 151 Table 108. Cross 47. —Rough A X rough C (tri); all mothers of tricolor stock except R175-4-toe. Rrss X RrSs = 3 Rss + 3 RSs + 2rr (3 A : 3 C : 2 Sm). No. 9 Rough A. d" Rough C. A B C D E Sm 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 R21 46-2 R23 46-2 R52 56-1 Do 1 1 1 1 2 1 1 2 R42 50-1 Do 1 R21 46-2 R23 46-2 R99 56-1 Do 1 2 1 1 R42 50-1 Do 3 3 1 4 R175 49-1 Do Total . . . 10 1 5 1 10 Table 109. Cross 48.— Rough A (Tri) X rough E (Tri). Rrss X RRSS = RSs (all C). No. 9 Rough A. cf Rough E. A B C D E Sm 1 R42 50-1 4003 Tri 2 1 Table 110. Cross 49. — Rough A (4-toe) X smooth (4-toe). Rrss X rrss = Rrss + rrss (1 A : 1 Sm). No. 9 Smooth. cf Rough A. A B C D E Sm 1 2 49 9 4-toe 5 9 9 4-toe Total 3922 4-toe.... 3609 4-toe.... 18 10 13 19 1 28 1 32 Table 111. Cross 50. — Rough A, B (tri) X smooth (4-toe etc.). Rough A, B with one or both of parents paitial rough. Expectation as in cross 49. No. 9 Smooth (or rough B). o" Rough A (or smooth). A B C D E Sm 1 2 3 4 79 9 Sm 4-toe R121 Sm 50-1. 3775 A Tri .Do . 13 14 3 3 R62 Sm 50-1 !R163 B 52-13 R22 A 46-2 99 Sm 4-toe 3 3 Total 19 20 *R163 may be RR. I 52 INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. Table 112. I St. -Rough A X smooth (tri); smooth with one or both parents partial rough. Rm x n6S = Rifla + rrfla (1 C: 1 Sm). Krss X rrSs = Rrss + RrSs + 2 rr (1 A : 1 C : 2 Sm). Krss X rrss = Rrss 4- rrss (,1 A : 1 Sm). (Smooth. cf Rough A. A B C D E Sm 1 ■_> a t 6 G R13 52-1 R123 ."-1-3 IU'l 54—3 R22 46-2 1 2 3 4 4 2 R76 4-toe . . Do 1 2 2 1 R133 47-2 . . Do R124 R133 .Do 8 1 R142 54-3 . . Do Total , 6 3 3 19 Table 113. Cross 52— Rough1 C, D (tri) X rough C (tri). RrSs X RrSs = 3 Rss 4- 6 RSs + 3 RSS + 4 rr (3 A : 6 C : 3 E : 4 Sm). No. l 2 3 4 5 6 7 6 9 10 11 12 13 11 15 9 Rough C. D. 3013 3246 Rll R54 3245 3939 3809 3246 3724 D 3724, 3246 R51 R57 R61, R57 R98 R103 Tri . Tri . 52-1 52-1 Tri . Tri . Tri . Tri . Tri . Tri . 56-1. 56-1. 56-1 56-1 56-1 cfRough C. 3780 Tri . . Do... Do . . . Do . . . 4019 Tri . . R58 52-5. Do... Do . . . Do . . . Do . . . R56 56-1. Do . . . Do . . . Do . . . Do . . . Total. 18 B 19 E 12 Sm 17 Remarks. Red-A Red-E, Red-Sm Red-A Rcd-C >A11 rough C except 3724. Table 114. Cross 63.— Rough C, D (tri) X rough E (tri). RrSs X RRSS = RSs + RSS (1 C: 1 E). or RRSsX RrSS = RSs + RSS (1 C : 1 E). or RrSa X RrSS = ■ 3 RSs + 3 RSS + 2 rr (3C:3E:2Sm) • 9 Rough C, D. cf Rough E. A B C D E Sm 1 a i R6D 54-15 R88C 52-1 R286C 62-6 R222 C 52 12. . . . 4003 Tri 1 2 2 R200 Hod 52-7 R280 52-14 3 1 Do 2 1 I •■• -il 4 1 6 1 TABLES. 153 Table 115. Cross 54- — Rough C, D (tri) X smooth (4-toe, etc.). RrSs X rrss = Rrss + RrSs + 2 rr (1 A : 1 C : 2 Sra). No. 9 Smooth (or rough C,D). cfRough CD (or smooth). A B C D E Sm 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 3 9 9 Sm 4-toe R62 Sm 50-1 3780 C Tri Do 1 1 8 2 17 14 6 9 2 5 9 9 Sm 4-toe R12 C 52-1 R26 D 54-15 R52 C 56-1 R102 C 56-1 R105 C 48-1 R106 C 48-1 . . Do 4 8 2 4 2 2 12 3 2 5 3 9 9 Sm 4-toe 5 4 9 9 Sm 4-toe . . , 5 9 9 Sm 4-toe 3 B31 Sm ld-9 M253Sm la-10 M255 Sm la-10 . 1 1 M380 Sm ^j . . .Do . 131, M253, M255 Sm . . .Do . 1 3 2 3 1 3 3 8 M384 Sep-Sm lb-9 . . R99 C 56-1 2 R62 Sm 50-1 R112 C 52-11.. 65 Sm 4-toe Do 1 5 1 3 1 1 3246 C Tri 2967 Sm BB Do 2 3809 C Tri 3724 D Tri . . Do . . 1 1 Total 34 29 13 1 79 Table 116. Cross 55 — Rough E (tri) X rough E (tri). RrSS X RrSS = 3 RSS + rrSS (3 E : 1 Sm). No. 9 Rough E. c? Rough E. A B C D E Sm 1 2 R221 52-12 1R201 Sm 52-7 R140 52-3 . . 2 2 3 .Do 4 3 xSee note, cross 57. Table 117. Cross 56.— Rough E (tri) X smooth (4-toe). RRSS X rrss = RrSs (all C) 1. RrSS X rrss = RrSs + rrSs (1 C : 1 Sm) 2-5. No. 9 Smooth (or rough E). c? Rough E (or smooth) . A B C D E Sm 1 2 3 4 5 3 9 9 Sm 4-toe 1 9 Sm 4-toe R^l Sm la 1 4003 E Tri 11 1 R140 E 52-3 3 2 1 2 !R201 Sm 52-7 .... R921 Fi 5*> 12 iq W-Sm 4-toe 1 Tntnl (1} 11 2 Tntnl (9 ^ 8 ^ee note, cross 57. l r,[ INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. Table 118. (Wot* 57. — Smooth (tri) X smooth (4-toe, etc.); both parents of tricolor smooths were partial roughs (cross 52). rr X rr = rr (all Sm). If, however, RSS, normally rough E, ie ever Sm: RrSS X rrss = RrSs + rrSs (1 C : 1 Sm). No. 9 Smooth. cfSmooth. A B C D E Sm 1 2 8 4 5 6 7 8 69 9 R131 52-4 14 2 6 2 3 3 2 1 RIBS 52-3 R13 52-1 99 4-toe ... Do R164 52-13 . . Do R 199 Red 52-7 R249 52-10 13 W 4-toe ... Do R263 52-11. .... Do 'R201 52-7. . . . Do 1 'R201 was called rough E? at birth with the note that there seemed to be a trace of roughness on ODe hind toe. No roughness was apparent when adult and she was called Sm, but nevertheless was tested by mating with a 4-toe smooth. The result shows that she was, genetically at least, like a rough E. Table 119. Cross 58. — Rough B, C (Lima) X rough B (Lima). No. 9 Rough B. c? Rough B. A B C D E Sm 1 2 3 4 5 6 L7 Lima . . . L97 60-6 L140 60-7 L5 Lima . . . L26 58-1 Do 2 1 1 3 2 1 1 2 1 1 7 (L97 60-6 .... \L81 Red 59-3 L99 Rough C 61-1 . . |L98 60-6 .... Do 2 4 7 2 2 1 1 LSI, L97, L99 (above) . Do 1 8 Total Table 120. Cross 69. — Rough A (Lima) X smooth (Lima). No. 9 Smooth (or rough A). cf Rough A (or smooth) . A B C D E Sm Remarks. 1 2 3 4 5 0 7 0 10 L18 8n 62-2.. I.ltSm 60-1 L9 A 58-1 . . Do 2 7 2 4 1 2 5 3 1 2 1 6 2 Red-B 2 Red-A, 2 Red-Sm Red-A Red-B 2 Sep(p)-Sm 2 Sep(p)-A 1 1 1 I-'lSro 60-2 Do . 3 126 Ba 58-1. Do . . L37 Sm 62-:< L678n 59 :•! Do 1 1 4 1 2 24 2 2 1 I.-'i. L67 8m (above) I -'_' A 60-2.. I >-.' A 50-3 Do LI Sm Lima . Do Ll00Sep(p)-A 61-1. • il L82 Sep(p)-Sm 59-8 . . 8 3 23 TABLES. 155 Table 121. Cross 60. — Rough B (Lima) X smooth (Lima). No. 9 Smooth. d" Rough B. A B C D E Sm Remarks. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 L4 Lima . L6 Sep(p) Lima . L5 Lima . Do 3 2 2 2 1 2 1 3 8 3 1 1 8 30 Red-A. 2 Red-B, Red-Sm Red-Sm L34 62-2 . . L37 62-3 . . L43 62-1 . . L26 58-1 Do 2 Do 1 2 2 L34, L43 (above) L41, L43 62-1.. Do Do L132 60-3 . . Do 1 1 1 8 L75 60-4 . . ..Do . (L14 60-1 . . \ L24 60-2 . . (L25 58-1 . . Total ) I-L131 60-3.. J 9 Table 122. Cross 61. — Rough C (Lima) X smooth (Lima). No. 9 Rough C. d" Smooth. A B C D E Sm Remarks. 1 L56 59-3 LI Lima. . . . 2 1 1 2 Sep(p)-A Table 123 Cross 62. — Smooth (Lima) X smooth (Lima). Offspring all smooth. No. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 9 Smooth. L3B L2 Red L8 Red L2, L8 Red LI 2 Red Do ... . L35 Red L17 Red L8 Red L8, L17 Red L64 Red L6 Sep(p) L19 Sep(p) L6 Sep(p) L19 Sep(p) Do ... . L58 Red(p) L121 Red(p) Do ... . L120 Red(p) Lima Lima Lima Lima 62-2. 62-3... 62-3 . . . Lima . . (above) 62-13. . Lima . . 62-3 . . . Lima . . 62-3 . . . 62-3 . . 62-11 62-11 cfSmooth. 62-2. 62-1. LI B Lima Do Do Do Lll Red L40 Red Do . . . Do .. . Do . . . Do . . . L59 Red LI B Do... L18 Sep(p) Do . . . L82 Sep(p) L59 (Red) Do L149 Red(p) 62-11. Do 62-3. Lima 62-3. 59-8. 62-3. B Red Sep(p) Red(p) 156 INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. Table 124. Cms* 68. — Rough A, B (Lima) X smooth (4-toe, etc.). No. 9 Rough A (or smooth). cTSmooth (or rough A). A B C D E Sm i a 3 i 5 L107 A 59-9.... I 10S \ 59-7 13W-Sm 4-toe.... . .Do 2 1 1 1 I 110 H 59-7 I),, 1 1 1 4 DIM) \Y Sm 18c-14. . I ) ' Hi \Y Sin 17d-7 L98 B 60-6 . 2 2 4 Do Total 4 Table 125. Cross 64. — Rough C (low grade due to C. rufescens) X smooth (4-toe, etc.). RrSs X rrss = Rrse + RrSs + 2 rr (1 A : 1 C : 2 Sm). x... 9 Rough C. cfSmooth. A B C D E Sm Remarks. 1 2 3 A606 Ag J A1687 64-1 . . A168SAg 65-1.. Total 166 4-toe... 2 2B-C B-Sm BrAgTb-A, 2 AgTb-A, 4 AgSrn 99 4-toe . . 1 4 5 AA83 g\ 3 3 2 Table 126. Cross 65. — Smooth (some C. rufescens blood) X rough A. rrSs X Rrss = Rrss + RrSs + 2rr (1 A : 1 C : 2 Sm). rrss X Rrss = Rrss X rrss (1 A : 1 Sm). No. 9 Smooth. cf Rough A. A B C D E Sm 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 A702 AgTb & V.05 J 2597 Ag stock Do 1 1 1 1 Mill' i Do. . . o 2 2 7 2 2 As42 J Do. . 3 A913A£Tb fa Do 0 9 9 AgTb tL-^J*. Do 12 4 5 B2 la-5 /B240 la-5.... K61, K62 7- 6 R88 52-1 . . \ Do / TABLES. 157 Table 127. Cross 66. — Rough A X smooth; both parents with a little C. mfescens blood. Rrss X rrss = Rrss + rrss (1 A : 1 Sm). No. 9 Rough A. c? Smooth. A B C D E Sm Remarks. 1 2 3 A1690 Ag 65-5 . . Do AA83 B-Sm & . . . M91 Ag-Sm 8-4 . . AA83 B-Sni & . . . 4 2 8 1 4 4 4 3 AgTb-A, Red-A, Ag Tb-B, AgLb-Sm, Ag Tb-Sm, 2 Red-Sm 2 AgLb-A, 2 AgLb-Sm, 2 AgTb-Sm 4 AgLb-A, 4 B-A, 2 Ag Lb-Sm, 2 B-Sm A1691 Ag 65-5. . Total . . 14 1 12 Table 128. Cross 67.— Rough A (4-toe, tri) X smooth (pure lea). Rrss X rrSS = RrSs + rrSs (1 C : 1 Sm). Young all light-bellied agouti. No. 9 Rough A. cf Smooth. A B C 3 2 D E Sm 1 2 3 4 /R215 49-1 \R252 49-2 .... R236 50-3 >724 SAa(R) lea .Do 1 1 2 4 R205 46-4 R213 49-1 5 Table 129. Cross 68.— Rough A X smooth (pure C. cutleri). Rrss X rrSS = RrSs + rrSs (1 C : 1 Sm). Young all light-bellied agouti. No. 9 Rough A. 3986 3988 3986, 3988 A1691 Ag AA567 Ag /AA568 \R80 Total . 4-toe . . 4-toe . . 4-toe . . 65-5. . 66-3 . . 66-3.. 54-15. cfSmooth. C128 Ag. .... Do . Do . Do . Do . Do B I) E Sm 15 158 INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. Table 130. -Rough C (tricolor) X smooth (pure C. cutlcri). RrSs X nSS = RrSs + RrSS + 2 rr (1 C : 1 E : 2 Sm). Young all light-bellied agouti. No 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 9 Black rough < '. D. cf Smooth. A B C D E Sm 2 1 2 1 1 2 3 12 3245 Tri R". » 5° 1 C128Ag .Do 1 1 1 2 2 1 1? 1 V 54—4 Do . •;■' i ri Rl l() .Do R 1 10 51 1 Do . R 1 70 47-3 .Do 1 H 1 1 52-1 Do R101 D 52-5 .Do . R154 D 54-4 Do 2 9 1 1 Table 131. Cross 70.— Rough A (guinea-pig) X rough C, D (|, \ cutleri). I X RrSs = Rss + RSs + 2rr (3 A : 3 C : 2 Sm) All | cutlcri Rough C, D, except K58, \ blood. R116 and R137 may be RRss. 9 Rough A (or C, D). cf Rough D(orA). A B C D E Sm Remarks. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 R 116 B-A 46-4.. R 117 B-A 46-4 K54 Ag-D 68-1 . . Do 2 1 Ag-A, B-A, Ag-C B-D, B-Sm Ag-A, 2 B-A, Ag-E 2 B-C, B-Sm 2 Ag-B B-A, Ag-B, Ag-E B-A, B-D Ag-A. 2 Ag-C, B-Sm B-C, B-Sm 1 1 R137B-\ 47-1 Do . 2 A. \608 B-A 66-3 Do O 1 1 K12Ag-D 68-3.. K14 Ag-D 68-3 R31 B-A 45-3.. 2 .Do 2 1 Do 3609 B-A 4-toe . R76B-A 45-4.. Do 1 1 1 2 1 8 1 2 1 2 1 1 4 Do K12 Ag-D 68-3 £58 B-C 70-5.. . . Do Total .... 8 3 Table 132. Cross 71. — Rough A (guinea-pig) X smooth (|, * cutleri). Rrffl X rrSs = Rrss + RrSs + 2rr (1 A : 1 C : 2 Sm). All \ cutlcri except K79, \ cutleri. No. 9 Smooth. d1 Rough A. A B C D E Sm Remarks. 1 8 I t. 7 8 K7Ag 77-1.. Klfi Ag 68 ■{ R31 B 45-3. . Do 3 3 2 1 2 o 7 9 1 3 1 1 Ag-A, 2 B-A, 2 B-B, B-C, 6 Ag-Sm, B-Sm Ag-A, 2 B-A, Ag-C. B-C, 3 Ag-Sm, 6 B-Sm 2 Ag-C, B-Sm 2 Ag-A, 2 Ag-Sm, B-Sm B-A, B-B, Ag-D, 2 B-C B-Sm Ag-C, Ag-Sm B-A, B-C K66 \.- 88-1 Do . . . K7, K.v> (above) K68 \ :' 77 1 Do Do 2 1 1 2 1 Kilo \, BS 8 Do K81 \j 89 l K7'.i B 7^ 1 Total. . . . 3609 H 4-toe 1 1 9 :•,'.»•_"_• B 4-toe . 1 10 3 1 22 TABLES. Table 133. Cross 72.— Smooth (guinea-pig) X rough C, D ($, \ cutleri). rrss X RrSs = Rrss + RrSs + 2rr (1 A : 1 C : 2 Sm). K71a, K92 may be RR. 159 No. 9 Rough C, D(orSm). d1 Smooth (or rough C, D). A 1 3 B C D 2 E Sm Remarks. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 K12 Ag-D 68-3 . . . K 14 Ag-D 68-3... 00Cr(Br)Sm Dil. ... Do 6 3 Ag-A, 2 B-D 2 Ag-A, B-A, 3 Ag-Sm, 3 B- Sm /B-C. 2 Ag-Sm, 1 B-Sm Ag-A, 2 Ag-C Ag-A, Sep-Sm 2 B-Sm,Sep-Sm Red-A, W-D, B-Sm B-A, Ag-Sm Ag-A, Ag-Sm Ag-A, B-A, Ag- C, 2 B-Sm Ag-Sm Ag-C, B-D /B-A, 3 AgTb- \ C+D,B-Sm B-Sm /K12 Ag-D 68-3 \A71a Ag-C 70-1 . . . K92Ag-C 70-9... 1 Do 1 2 / Do K157Ag-C 72-12.. K147 AgTb-D 72-13 . . 13Cr(Br)SmDil.... Do 1 3 1 1 1 2 1 1 1 21 K142 B-D 72-1 . . . D40Cr(Br)Sm 36-1... R173 B-Sm 49-1 . . . 00Cr(Br)SmDil... . K60 Ag-C 71-2 . . Do 2 1 20 B-Sm 4-toe . . AA533 Ag-Sm 66-2 . . . AA586 Ag-Sm 106-10 . K54 Ag-D 68-1 . . K56 B-C 70-5 . . 1 Do 1 1 1 2 /M382AgTb-Sm 16-9. . . \B239 AgTb-Sm la-5. . . B239 AgTb-Sm la-5 . . . Do 1 Dn Total 12 6 6 Table 134. Cross 78. — Smooth (4-toe) X rough A, B (| cutleri). rrss X Rrss = Rrss 4- rrss (1 A : 1 Sm). No. 9 Rough A, B. d1 Smooth. A B C D E Sm Remarks. 1 2 K95 B-B 71-1 /K101 B-A 70-8 \K106B-A 71-2 Total . . 99 B 4-toe 1 1 2 B-Sm 2 B-A, B-Sm |l3 W 4-toe 2 2 Table 135. Cross 74- — Smooth (f, f cutleri) X rough A (? cutleri). rrSs X Rrss = Rrss + RrSs + 2rr (1 A : 1 C : 2 Sm). rrss X Rrss = Rrss + rrss (1 A : 1 Sm). No. 9 Smooth. cf Rough A. A B C D E Sm Remarks. 1 2 K68 Ag 77-1 K42 B 78-2 K59 B 71-2 1 B-C B-Sm .Do 1 1 Total 1 160 INHERITANCE IN GUINEA-PIGS. Table 136. Cross 75. — Rough A, B (J culleri) X rough A (J cutleri). Rres X Rres = 3 Rss + rres (3 A : 1 Sm). No. 9 Rough A, B. d* Rough A. A B C D E Sm Remarks. 1 2 E60Ag-B 70-6 K53 B 71-1 . K59B 71-2 Do 1 2 3 B-A 2B-A Total Table 137. Cross 76. — Rough C (f cutleri) X rough C (£ cutleri). RSs X RSs = Rss + 2 RSs + RSS (1 A : 2 C : 1 E : ? Sm ?). No. 9 Rough C. cT Rough C. A B C D E Sm Remarks. 1 K114B 74-1.... K93 Ag 70-9 . . 1 3 1 /Ag-C, Ag-D, 2 B-D. 1 Ag-E Table 138. Cross 77. — Black (BB) X agouti (pure cutleri). Parents and offspring all smooth. No. 9 Black. cf Agouti. AgLb Black W 1 2 B 9 9 BB C128Ag pureC... 6 Table 139. Cross 78.— Black (BW) X agoutf (§, \, \ cutleri). Expectation : 1 Ag : 1 black : some white. Parents and offspring all smooth. No. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 9 Black (or agouti). I'd1 Agouti (or black). AgLb Black W G B 9 9 BW 2 B 9 9 BW 3B 9 9 BW K20Ag(}) 78-1.... K22A*(« 78-1.... C67 Ag K4 Ag (i) K-M Ag(I) 39 B BW . . (J) 78-1 .... 78-1 .... 12 6 9 3 1 3 12 9 5 6 Do . . 3 2 4 1 5 1 3 K29Ag(*) 78-1.... Do . . K103Ag(i) 78-4.... Do . . K66Ag(|) 78-2.... Do . . . E109Ag (I) 78-6.... Do . . 1 3 H 9 9 BW Total K104Ag(i) 78-4 . . 35 41 10 PART III FURTHER STUDIES OF PIEBALD RATS AND SELECTION, WITH OBSERVATIONS ON GAMETIC COUPLING By W. E. CASTLE THE PROGENY OF HOODED RATS TWICE CROSSED WITH WILD RATS. In 1914 Castle and Phillips published a report on breeding experi- ments with hooded rats, in which it was shown that the hooded color pattern — itself a Mendelian recessive character in crosses with the entirely colored (or "self") coat of wild rats — is subject to quantitative variation, and that different quantitative conditions of the hooded pattern are heritable. (Compare fig. 36, plate 7.) It was also shown that by repeated selection of the more extreme variations in the hooded pattern (either plus or minus) it is possible gradually to modify the racial mean, mode, and range as regards these fluctuations, without eliminat- ing further fluctuation or greatly reducing its amount. We concluded that the unit character, hooded color pattern, is a quantitatively vary- ing one, but were at that time unable to decide whether the observed variability was due simply and exclusively to variation in a single Mendelian unit factor or partly to independent and subsidiary modify- ing Mendelian factors. Since publication of the above I have been engaged in further experi- ments designed to show which of the alternative explanations is the correct one, and these are now sufficiently advanced to indicate definite conclusions. Previous experiments had shown that when a race of hooded rats, whose character has been modified by selection (either plus or minus), is crossed with wild rats, the extracted hooded animals obtained in F2 as recessives show regression toward the mean condition of the recessive race before selection began. This result suggested that the regression observed might be due to removal by the cross of modifying factors, which selection had accumulated in the hooded race. If this view was correct, it was thought that further crossing of the extracted hooded animals with the same wild race should result in further regression, and that if this further regression was not observed a different explanation must be sought for the regression already noted. The entire experiment has accordingly been repeated from the beginning, with the same result as regards regression in the first F, generation, but with no regression of the same sort in a second F2 con- taining twice-extracted hooded animals. So far from observing further regression as a result of the second cross with wild rats, we have unmis- takable evidence that the movement of the mean, mode, and range of the hooded character has been in the reverse direction. So the hypothe- sis of modifying factors to account for the regression and for the pro- gressive changes observed under selection becomes untenable. In repeating the experiment of crossing hooded rats of our selected races with wild rats, great care has been taken to employ as parents individuals of the greatest racial purity and to inbreed the offspring 163 164 INHERITANCE IN RATS. brother with sister, thus precluding the possibility of introducing modifying factors from other sources. In making the second set of crosses, the extracted individual has, wherever possible, been crossed with its own wild grandparent. In the few cases in which this was impossible, wild animals of the same stock have been used. This stock consisted of a colony of wild rats which invaded the basement of the Bussey Institution apparently from a nearby stable. Owing to faulty construction of the building they were able to breed in spots inaccessible to us, and it took many months of continuous and persistent trapping to secure their extermination. During this period we trapped a hun- dred or more of them, all typical Norway rats, colored all over, without even the white spot occasionally seen on the chest of wild rats. Two genei ations of rats from this wild stock have been reared in the labora- tory, and all have this same self-colored condition. The hooded animals used in the experiments to be reported on in this connection consisted of 4 individuals of the plus-selected series, a male and 3 females, as follows: Table 140. Individual. Grade.1 Generation. 9 5513.... c?6348 9 6600 .... 9 6955 +4| +4 +4* +4 10 10 12 12 'See figure 35, plate 7, for significance of the grades. Each of these animals was mated with a single wild mate, and their children were weaned directly into breeding cages containing a male and two or three females (brother and sisters). In the case of two inatings, Fx males of the same parentage were at the time lacking and 1 1 1 ales from a different cross were used. The results of such matings are tabulated by themselves and serve a useful purpose as controls. The Fx animals all closely resembled their wild parents, but many of them had a white spot on the chest. They ranged from grade +5j to +6 (self). The F2 animals are classified in table 141, where it appears that 73 of them were hooded and 219 non-hooded (i. e., like Fi), an exact 1 : 3 ratio. More than half of this F2 generation consists of the grand- children of 9 5513, produced by breeding her children brother with sister, those children all having been sired by the same wild rat. Her grandchildren include 41 hooded and 107 non-hooded young. The hooded young range in grade from +1| to +4, their mean grade being +3.05, a considerable regression from the grade of the grandmother, which was 4.26. Hooded rats of the same grade and generation as the grandmother, when bred with each other, produced young of mean grade +3.84. HOODED CROSSED WITH WILD. 165 (See table 10, Castle and Phillips.) The mean of the extracted hooded grandchildren in this case (being 3.05) shows a regression of 0.79 from that expected for the uncrossed hooded race. From the extracted hooded grandchildren of 9 5513, produced as just described by a cross with a wild male, 7 individuals, 2 males and 5 females, were selected for a second cross with the wild race. They ranged in grade from +2 to +3*. (See table 142.) They produced several litters of young of the same character as the first Fx young, all being similar to wild rats in appearance, except for the frequent occurrence of a white spot on the belly. These second Fx young were at weaning time mated, brother with sister, in breeding-pens, precisely as had been done with the first F/s. They produced 394 second F2 young, of which 98 were hooded and 296 non-hooded, a perfect 1 : 3 ratio. The hooded young varied in grade from +2 to +4, as shown in table 142, the data there being given for each family separately as well as for all combined in the totals. One family was very like another as regards the character of the hooded young, except that the higher-grade grandparents had grandchildren of slightly higher grade. Thus the average of all the 98 hooded young was +3.47, but the average of those descended from the 3 grandparents of lowest grade was less than this, while the average of those descended from the 3 grandparents of highest grade was greater. This is just what had been observed throughout the entire selection experiments. (See Castle and Phillips.) If we weight each of the grandparents in table 142 in proportion to the number of its hooded grandchildren, then the mean grade of all the grandparents is +2.95. Since the mean grade of all the 41 first F2 hooded grandchildren, from which these 7 were chosen, was +3.05, it will be seen that these 7 are, so far as grade is concerned, fair repre- sentatives of the 41, being in fact of slightly lower mean grade. It is therefore all the more striking that their grandchildren, the second F2 hooded young (table 142) , are of higher grade. They regress in an oppo- site direction to that taken by the first F2 hooded young. Thus the original hooded ancestor ( 9 5513) was of grade 4.25. The grade of hooded young expected from such animals is 3.84. What she produced in F2, following a cross with the wild male, was young of mean grade 3.05^ Seven of these of mean grade 2.95 produced a second F2 contain- ing hooded young of mean grade 3.47. This is a reversed regression of 0.52 on the grade of their actual hooded grandparents, or of 0.42 on the group from which their grandparents were chosen. Their mean lies about midway1 between that which would have been expected from the original hooded female (5513) had no crossing with wild rats occurred and that which was observed in the first F2. iln The Scientific Monthly (Jan. 1916) I have stated that a second cross showed "a return to about what the selected race would have been had no crossing at all occurred This is obviously inaccurate and should be corrected. It rests on a comparison with the combined average of both the older and the more recent experiments. ],,i; INHERITANCE IN RATS. Obviously these facte do not harmonize with the assumption that the repression observed in the first F2 was due to loss of modifying fac- tors accumulated during the ten preceding generations of selection; for no further loss occurs in the second F2. On the other hand, a part ial recovery is made of what was lost in the first F2. This suggests the idea that that loss may have been due to physiological causes non- genet ic in character, such as produce increased size in racial crosses; for among guinea-pigs (as among certain plants) it has been found that Fx has :m increased size due to vigor produced by crossing and not due to heredity at all. This increased size persists partially in F2, but for the most pari is not in evidence beyond Fx. I would not suggest that the present case is parallel with this, but it seems quite possible that similar non-genetic agencies are concerned in the striking regression of the first 1 and the subsequent reversed regression in the second F2. Whatever its correct explanation may be, the fact of the reversed regression in a second F2 is very clear, as other cases than those already iliscussed will show. A hooded rat of grade +4 and generation 10, c?6348, had by a wild female several young of the character already described for the young of 9 5513. These, mated brother with sister, produced a first F2 (table 141) of 90 rats, 22 of which were hooded, 68 being non-hooded, again a good 1 : 3 ratio. The hooded young ranged from +2 to +4 in grade, their mean being 3.28. Of the 22 hooded individuals, 1 male and 7 females were mated with wild rats to obtain a second Fi, and the second Fx animals were then mated brother with sister to obtain the desired second F2. The character of this is shown family by family in table 143. It contained 497 individuals, of which 121 were hooded and 376 non-hooded, a ratio of 1 : 3.1. The weighted mean of the 8 selected grandparents is 2.93, which is 0.35 below the mean of the 22 first F2 hooded animals which they represent. The mean of the second I hooded young is 3.22, which indicates a reversed regression of 0.29 on the grade of the grandparents, but shows no significant difference from the mean of the grandparental group (3.28). All except one of the 8 families classified in table 143 show unmis- takably the reversed regression. This exceptional family consists of the grandchildren of 9 9747. They have a mean grade of 2.90, sub- stantially the same as that of the entire group of grandparents but con- siderably lower than that of their own hooded grandmother. Appa- rently she did not come up genetically to her phenotypic grade. This the other grandparents of the group did. For those of lowest grade 2|) produced lower-grade hooded grandchildren than did the grand- parent > of highest grade (3-|, 4), as was found to be the case also in table L42. We may next trace the inheritance of the hooded character through a third but smaller family produced by two successive crosses with wild HOODED CROSSED WITH WILD. 167 rats, the hooded character in this case being derived from 9G955, grade +4, generation 12. The character of her first F2 descendants is shown in table 141. They consist of 5 hooded and 27 non-hooded individuals. The mean grade of the hooded young is 3.51, but the number of these young is too small to make this mean of much signifi- cance. One of the hooded young (d"9660,+3f) was mated with a wild female to secure a second F: generation and from this in due course was produced the second F2 generation (table 144) . It consisted of 21 hooded and 44 non-hooded young. The hooded young showed the usual range (2 to 4). Their mean grade was 3.50, substantially identical with that of the first F2 animals, but 0.25 below that of the actual hooded grand- parent. This family history is less satisfactory than the two already discussed because of the smaller numbers which it includes. It con- tains nothing contradictory to the interpretation already given, though reversed regression is not in this case in evidence. In two cases Fi females could not be mated with brothers and so mates were taken from other families. Thus "mixed Fi matings" were made between children of 5513 and 6600 and children of 5513 and 6955. (See table 141.) The former mating produced 3 hooded and 12 non-hooded " first" F2 young; the latter produced 2 hooded and 5 non-hooded " first" F2 young. The grade of the hooded young pro- duced by these mixed matings was not different from that of brother- sister matings, so far as the small numbers permit one to judge. One of these mixed matings was carried into a second F2 generation. The first F2 hooded ^9711, +3|, was mated with a wild female, and the young were bred, brother with sister, producing 16 hooded and 33 non- hooded young. (See table 144.) The mean grade of the 16 hooded young was 3.28, nearly the same as that of the first F2 hooded grand- parent. No additional regression through loss of modifiers (or other agency) is here in evidence. The result is the same as that observed in families wholly unmixed. The attention of my pure-line critics, who think that in our mass-selection experiments insufficient attention has been given to individual pedigrees, is particularly directed to the foregoing case. Having now discussed each family history separately, we may com- bine all the second F2 families in one table, in order to get a clearer impression of the results as a whole. (See table 145.) The second F2 generation thus combined includes 256 hooded and 749 non-hooded individuals, a ratio of 1 : 2.9, an unmistakable mono-hybrid Mendelian ratio. The mean grade of the hooded individuals is 3.34. The weighted mean grade of their hooded grandparents was 3.02, which indicates a reversed regression of 0.32 for the entire second F2 group of hooded animals. Classified according to the grade of the (first F2) grandparent, they show a correlation between grade of grandparent and grade of grand- 108 INHERITANCE IN RATS. child. The 1«>\\ er-grade grandparent has lower-grade hooded grand- children, and the higher-grade grandparent has higher-grade hooded grandchildren. This shows that the variation in grade is (in part at least) -over gametes pro- gametes produc- gametes. gametes. ducing yellows. ing black-eyed. 1: 1 50 42.9 55.6 1: 2 33.3 29.4 36.8 1:3 25 22.6 27.3 1:4 20 18.4 21.6 1:4-4 18.5 17.1 19.8 L:fi 16.7 15.5 17.8 1:6 14.3 13.4 15.2 TABLES. 181 TABLES. Table 141 shows the classification of extracted hooded first F2 young obtained from crossing hooded rats of the plus-selected series with wild rats. Table 141. Hooded grandparents. Grade of hooded grandchildren. Total hooded. Total non- hooded. Means of hooded. 1* 1 ;1 2 2\ 01 •"2 2| 3 31 3* •J3 4 9 5513, +4J, gen. 10 1 3 1 2 1 1 7 2 8 4 6 3 1 1 11 5 4 1 1 1 12 7 6 3 16 1 1 2 41 22 5 3 2 107 68 27 12 5 3.05 3.28 3.51 3.17 3.37 cf6348, +4, gen. 10 9 6955, +4, gen. 12 9 5513, +41, and 9 6600, +41, gen. 12 2 9 5513, +41, and 9 6955, +4, gen. 12 Totals ] 4 2 2 9 14 73 219 3.17 Table 142 shows the classification of extracted hooded second F2 young obtained from crossing first F2 hooded rats (table 141) with wild rats. The hooded grandparents were themselves grandchildren of 95513, +4J, generation 10, on the side of both parents. Table 142. Hooded grand- parents. Grade of hooded grandchildren. Total hooded. Total non- hooded. Means of hooded. 2 21 o| 03 "4 3 31 o2 3f 4 9 9619, +2 c?9686, +2f... 99620, +2f... 99729, +2| d*9727, +3 9 9728, +3 1 1 1 2 1 1 2 1 1 1 3 1 1 1 2 1 2 5 1 1 2 2 4 11 6 2 1 3 4 7 8 7 1 4 1 5 2 5 13 10 30 22 16 8 28 24 22 104 68 42 3.37 3.40 3.06 3.62 3.47 3.55 3.70 9 9621, +31 Totals . . . 1 2 3 4 6 13 28 30 11 98 296 3.47 Table 143 shows the classification of extracted hooded second F2 young obtained from crossing first F2 hooded rats (table 141) with wild rats. The hooded grandparents were themselves grandchildren of (^6348, +4, generation 10, on the side of both parents. Table 143. Hooded grand- parents. Grade of hooded grand- children. Total hooded. Total non- hooded. Means of hooded. 1 2 ■■■4 2 21 24 2f 3 31 31 3f 1 0^9639, +2 9 9704, +2f . . . . 1 2 1 7 1 1 1 1 3 1 2 1 1 6 1 7 2 5 2 4 1 1 1 1 15 4 1 4 5 4 2 6 4 2 8 3 1 1 1 2 2 1 39 6 1 27 16 21 9 2 110 16 10 76 47 74 40 3 3.24 3.17 3.50 2.90 3.28 3.48 3.36 3.87 9 9765, +3 9 9747, +31 ... . 9 9703, +3|. . . . 1 9 9705, +3| 9 9748, +31 9 9796, +4 Totals 2 in 2 2 8 23 8 :;.-, 24 7 121 376 3.22 1S2 INHERITANCE IN RATS. Table 1 11 shows the chissification of extracted hooded second Fs young obtained from crossing first Fj hooded rats with wild rats. The hooded grandparent, d"9660, +3$, was I grandson of v 8965, +4, generation 12, on the side of both parents. The hooded grand- parent, d"9711, + 3i, was a grandson, on the side of one parent, of 9 5513, +4J-, generation 10, and on the side of the other parent, of 96955, +4, generation 12. (See table 141.) Table 144. Hooded grand- parents. Grade of hooded grandchildren. Total hooded. Total non- hooded. Means of hooded. 2 -•; i i 9J - « 1 2 3 3 1 2 3 31 2 4 6 n 35 4 cf9660, +3 J... c?9711. +31. .. 1 5 4 9 9 2 1 21 16 44 33 3.50 3.28 Totals 1 11 3 37 77 3.40 Table 145 is a combination of tables 142 to 144, in which the second F2 young are classi- fied according to the grade of their first F2 hooded grandparent. Table 145. Grade of hooded grand- parents. Grade of hooded grand- children. Total hooded. Total non- hooded. Means of hooded. H 2 21 2] 2 i 3 33 3i 31 4 2 n 3 31 31 31 4 l l 2 2 2 7 1 12 1 1 1 1 4 2 1 2 1 6 3 1 2 4 4 1 15 6 3 4 9 9 1 32 5 4 7 6 3 2 27 16 12 18 10 11 5 6 8 IS 13 13 9 1 1 1 5 7 4 2 1 41 34 53 59 46 21 2 118 90 182 151 161 44 3 3.25 3.29 3.48 3.22 3.39 3.50 3.87 3.02 72 65 21 256 749 3.34 Table 146 shows the classification of generation 12, plus-selection series. enlargement of table 12 of Castle and Phillips. Table 146. This is an Grade of parents. Grade of offspring. Totals. Means. Regres- sion. 21 2 J 2J 3 31 4 31 20 4 7 H 41 i; 5 5j 31 4 35 3.83 -.08 3J 3 23 21 6 2 2 57 3.94 -.06 4 1 2 12 62 66 L2 6 1 1 2 164 3.91 .09 H o 3 26 106 91 30 6 3 1 267 3.87 .25 41 1 11 25 35 16 5 2 95 3.95 .30 4| 3 6 14 10 7 3 1 1 45 4.17 .20 41 1 •I 1 1 7 4.14 .36 4f 1 3 4 11 3.91 .71 41 4J 5 2 3 5 68 246 242 B2 26 1 12 4 3 1 4.75 .25 4.Ki 682 3.93 .17 TABLES. 183 Table 147 shows the classification of generation 13, plus-selection series. This is an enlarge- ment of table 13 of Castle and Phillips. Table 147. Grade of offspring. G'ade of parents. Totals. Means. Regres- sion. 21 3 ol 35 31 4 41 1 1 ' 2 41 5 ->.; 3* 1 1 1 3 3.50 0 3* 3i 1 3 11 3 1 1 1 21 4.08 -.33 31 1 2 4 9 7 4 1 3 31 3.90 -.03 4 1 7 33 60 59 25 17 2 1 205 3.90 .10 4* 1 11 32 33 13 5 1 96 3.92 .20 4i 1 7 23 33 13 1 2 80 3.93 .32 4| 1 2 8 7 6 1 3 i 29 4.03 .34 4* 1 15 17 10 5 1 1 50 4.05 .45 4| 1 2 1 4 4.00 .62 41 3 1 1 5 3.95 .80 41 1 2 1 1 5 4.05 .82 4.13 1 3 11 61 155 172 76 32 13 ■l i 529 3.94 .19 Table 148 6hows the classification of generation 14, plus-selection series. Table 148. Grade of offspring. Grade of Totals. Means. Regres- parents. sion. n 2? 3 33 3* 32 4 41 H 41 5 •:>! H 3* 2 6 3 1 12 3.83 -.33 3| 2 9 4 7 l 1 24 4.02 -.39 3! i 2 1 11 32 45 18 5 115 3.90 -.15 31 1 28 52 63 28 8 3 1 184 3.97 - .10 4 2 4f 84 122 50 6 2 306 3.92 .08 4| 19 74 72 56 15 3 1 l 241 3.99 .13 41 6 25 48 42 8 1 130 4.04 .21 41 1 2 24 36 29 6 2 100 4.04 .33 4* 3 12 31 37 12 3 2 l 101 4.16 .34 41 11 13 15 11 5 2 l 58 4.23 .39 41 5 16 14 8 2 45 4.17 .58 41 5 51 1 7 14 6 3 1 l 33 4.33 .54 1 4 1 6 4.25 .87 51 3 1 1 1 l 4 4 4.75 .50 4.14 i 4 113 335 461 315 89 24 9 i 1,359 4.01 .13 [84 INHERITANCE IN RATS. Table 148 Axmt the Hassification of generation 15, plus-aelection series. Table 149. ( trade of ( irade of offspring. Totals. Means. Regres- sion. 2 J 3 31 31 3i 4 41 41 41 5 5J 51 3| 31 4 41 41 41 41 4| 4| 41 5 51 51 1 1 1 l 2 1 2 1 1 1 3 5 16 29 22 18 9 3 3 3 10 58 184 L83 207 99 37 25 13 1 7 30 156 670 721 969 506 237 168 146 43 18 19 3.57 3.80 3.91 4.00 4.02 4.06 4.10 4.11 4.22 4.37 4.27 4.37 4.36 .18 .07 .09 .12 .23 .31 .40 .51 .53 .50 .73 .75 .89 11 46 255 296 357 159 87 38 25 9 4 2 2 28 165 165 290 175 88 58 42 21 4 10 7 29 44 71 48 14 24 34 10 7 5 3 8 L'l 6 5 5 15 2 3 1 1 1 3 7 2 9 12 1 1 6 4 2 1 1 4.38 1 2 9 108 820 1,289 1,048 293 69 36 11 4 3,690 4.07 .31 Table 150 shows the classification of generation 16, plus-selection seiies. Table 150. Grade of parents. Grade of offspring. Totals. Means. Regres- sion. 31 31 3J 4 41 41 4f o bj 5* bi Os 41 41 4| 41 4| 4} 41 5 51 1 ■i 4 g 7 26 34 149 25 12 64 64 316 82 50 12 23 8 1 37 40 271 69 61 16 26 25 8 8 5 58 18 26 19 8 15 6 1 9 5 11 10 o o 7 1 4 6 2 2 1 1 2 1 1 1 2 1 1 2 1 1 139 149 816 206 166 66 69 61 18 4.04 4.02 4.08 4.10 4.24 4.47 4.21 4.44 4.39 .08 .23 .29 .40 .38 .38 .66 .56 .73 5 1 4.45 1 252 620 553 163 46 16 9 4 1 1,690 4.13 .32 Table 151 shows the classification of generation 13, minus-selection series, enlargement of table 28 of Castle and Phillips. Table 151. This is an Grade of offspring (minus). Grade of Totals. Means. Regres- parents. sion. u 2 21 21 2| 3 31 31 -2J 7 i:: 25 L2 1 116 2.25 0 8 71 80 ^7 56 19 5 329 2.39 0 -21 8 65 65 92 46 11 2 1 290 2.38 .12 -2| 3 33 58 n 4 4 1 170 2.50 .12 1 5 •4 16 L2 1 1 46 2.56 .19 -21 i 8 10 7 2 34 2.42 .45 -3 4 1 216 6 297 5 182 3 2 21 2.59 .41 -2 27 221 47 1 i 2 1,006 2.40 .09 TABLES. 185 Table 152 shows the classification of generation 14, minus-selection series. Table 152. Grade of parents. Grade of offspring (minus). Totals. Means. Regres- sion. 1 H li 14 2 2| 21 2f 3 31 31 -2i 2 2 14 8 26 2.54 -.29 -2| 2 8 20 22 11 2 65 2.40 -.03 -21 1 3 40 50 73 29 2 1 199 2.36 .14 -2| 2 23 32 59 44 10 1 1 172 2.44 .18 -2f 7 20 42 43 11 4 127 2.60 .15 -21 5 10 25 14 2 2 58 2.52 .35 -3 1 1 15 8 o o 5 33 2.69 .31 -31 6 3 7 4 20 2.56 .56 -3J 4 3 7 2.85 .40 -3| — — O 4 3 10 2.75 .62 -2.64 1 7 86 141 256 172 10 13 1 717 2.48 .16 Table 153 shows the classification of generation 15, minus-selection series. Table 153. Grade of offspring (minus). Grade of parents. Totals. Means. Regres- sion. 1 a 1 4 2 21 21 2f 3 31 31 -21 -21 1 11 4 23 4 15 4 13 13 64 2.46 2.39 -.33 -.14 2 -21 1 15 24 47 29 3 119 2.45 -.07 -21 2 39 65 102 68 14 290 2.49 .01 _9£ z8 1 41 97 137 99 24 6 2 407 2.50 .12 "4 7 37 91 70 31 3 1 240 2.60 .15 _9i 4 18 62 70 22 7 183 2.64 .23 -3 1 17 16 13 4 51 2.76 .24 -3| 4 11 17 7 1 2 42 2.73 .34 -31 5 12 8 o 1 29 2.85 .60 -2.65 4 118 273 191 398 124 24 6 1,438 2.54 .11 Table 154 shows the classificatioi of generation 16, minus-selection series. Table 154. Grade of offspring (minus). Grade of Totals. Means. Regres- parents. sion. 1 u u 1 3 1 4 2 21 21 •21 3 31 31 Q8 •J 4 4 -21 3 5 1 9 2.19 .06 -2| 1 5 4 3 1 14 2.16 .21 -21 4 27 61 56 3 1 152 2.55 -.05 -2f 1 16 56 188 148 36 5 450 2.58 .04 -2f 1 10 36 130 151 28 5 1 362 2.62 .13 -21 1 11 45 187 230 71 16 1 1 563 2.66 .21 -3 2 12 95 128 65 12 1 1 316 2.73 .27 -31 6 30 36 15 10 1 98 2.74 .38 -31 12 3 1 16 2.83 .42 -2.79 1 3 51 191 695 762 221 50 4 1 1 1,980 2.63 .16 I Mi INHERITANCE IN RATS. Tat le 155 &SM* the .-luesification of generation 17, minus-s* lection Feries. Table 155. < '. rade of parents. Grade of offspring (minus). Totals. Means. Regres- sion. 1 2 2 2 4 -1 in 12 34 3 2J 40 77 110 28 5 3 2| 49 81 145 42 18 16 3 11 2S 51 18 17 15 1 3i 1 1 19 7 6 6 34 1 1 1 1 32 4 41 1 -2| -2| -21 -3 -3| -3i -3f 113 202 364 98 48 41 2 -2.63 -2.65 -2.71 -2.75 -2.92 -2.92 -3.25 0 .10 .16 .25 .20 .33 .12 -2.86 1 8 59 263 351 141 to 4 1 868 -2.70 .16 Table 156 summarizes the results of the \ lus-selection of hooded rats continued through sixteen successive generations. Table 150. Genera- tion. Mean grade of parents. Mean grade of offspring. Lowest grade of offspring. Highest grade of offspring. Standard deviation of offspring. Correla- tion, parents- offspring. Number of offspring. 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 2.51 2.52 2.73 3.09 3.33 3.52 3.56 3.75 3.78 3.88 3 98 4.10 4.13 4.14 4.38 4.45 2.05 1.92 2.51 2.73 2.90 3.11 3.20 3.48 3.54 3.73 3.78 3.92 3.94 4.01 4.07 4.13 + 1.00 -1.00 + .75 + .75 + .75 + 1.50 + 1.50 + 1.75 + 1.75 +2.25 +2.75 +2.25 +2.75 +2.75 +2.50 +3.25 +3.00 +3.75 +4.00 +3.75 +4.25 +4.50 +4.75 +4.50 +4.50 +5.00 +5.00 +5.25 +5.25 +5.50 +5.50 +5.87 .54 .73 .53 .47 .50 .49 .55 .44 .35 .36 .29 .31 .34 .34 .29 .29 .29 .31 .33 .06 .16 .18 .21 .09 .21 .11 .23 .16 .13 .31 .30 .31 150 471 341 444 610 861 1,077 1,408 1 , 322 776 697 682 529 1.359 3,690 1,690 Total 16,107 TABLES. 187 Table 157 summarizes tbe results of the minus-selection of hooded rats continued through seventeen successive generations. Table 157. Genera- tion. Mean grade of parents. Mean grade of offspring. Lowest grade of offspring. Highest grade of offspring. Standard deviation of offspiing. Correla- tion, parents- offspring. Number of offspring. 1 -1.46 -1.00 + .25 -2.00 .51 55 2 -1.41 -1.07 + .50 -2.00 .49 -.03 132 3 -1.56 -1.18 0 -2.00 .48 .20 195 4 -1.69 -1.28 + .50 -2.25 .46 .02 329 5 -1.73 -1.41 0 -2.50 .50 .18 701 6 -1.86 -1.56 0 -2.50 .44 .16 1,252 7 -2.01 -1.73 0 -2.75 .35 .14 1,680 8 -2.05 -1.80 0 -2.75 .28 .09 1,726 9 -2.11 -1.92 - .50 -2.75 .28 .05 1,591 10 -2.18 -2.01 -1.00 -3.25 .24 .15 1,451 11 -2.30 -2.15 -1.00 -3.50 .35 .08 984 12 -2.44 -2.23 -1.00 -3.50 .37 .40 1,037 13 -2.48 -2.39 -1.75 -3.50 .34 .18 1,006 14 -2.64 -2.48 -1.00 -3.50 .30 .28 717 15 -2.65 -2.54 -1.75 -3.50 .29 .35 1,438 16 -2.79 -2.63 -1.00 -4.00 .27 .26 1,980 17 -2.86 -2.70 -1.75 -4.25 .28 .22 868 Total. .. 17,142 Table 158 shows the classification of the F2 young obtained by crossing homozygous "mutant" with wild rats. Table 158. Mutant grandparents. Grade of off- spring. Totals. 5 5i :>l 5! 5| 6 9 0630, +5 i 3 2 12 22 \2 3 9 29 11 1 2 1 20 59 19 5 46 114 42 10 90698, +5* 90694, +5i 1 9 0630, +5i, or 0636, +5| 1 Total 1 6 49 50 3 103 212 BIBLIOGRAPHY. Allen, G. M. 1914. Pattern development in mammals and birds. Amer. Nat., 48, pp. 385-412; 467-484; 550-566. 1903. The present state of knowledge of color-heredity in mice and rats. Proc. Zool. Soc, vol. 2, pp. 71-78. 1913. Mendel's principles of heredity. Univ. Press, Cambridge, xiv + 413 pp., 38 figs., 6 pis. Hat R, E. 1911. Einfuhrung in die experimentelle Vererbungslehre. Berlin, Gebriider Born- traeger, iv + 293 pp., 80 figs., 9 pis. Calkins, G. N., and Louise H. Gregory. 1913. Variations in the progeny of a single exconjugant of Paramecium caudatum. Jour. Exi . Zool., 15, pp. 467-525. Castle, W. E. 1905. Heredity of coat characters in guinea-pigs and rabbits. Carnegie Inst. Wash. Pub. No. 23, 78 pp., 6 pis. 1906. The origin of a polydactylous race of guinea-pigs. Carnegie Inst. Wash. Pub. No. 49, 29 pp. 1907. On a case of reversion induced by cross breeding and its fixation. Science, n. s., 25, pp. 151-153. 1907a. Color varieties of the rabbit and of other rodents : Their origin and inheritance. Science, n. s., 26, pp. 287-291. 1908. A new color variety of the guinea-pig. Science, d. s., 28, pp. 250-252. 1912. Or the origin of an albino race of deer mouse. Science, n. s., 35, pp. 346-348. 1912a. On the origin of a pink-eyed guinea-pig with colored coat. Science, n. s., 35, pp. 508-510. 1912b. On the inheritance of the tricolor coat in guinea-pigs and its relation to Galton's law of ancestral heredity. Amer. Nat., 46, pp. 437-440. 1913. Reversion in guinea-pigs and its explanation. Carnegie Inst. Wash. Pub. No. 179, pp. 1-10. 1914. Size inheritance and the pure line theory. Zeit. f. ind. Abst. u. Vererbungs- lehre, 12, pp. 225-237. 1914a. Some new varieties of rats and guinea-pigs and their relation to problems of color inheritance Amer. Nat., 48, pp. 65-73. 1915. Some experiments in mass selection. Amer. Nat., 49, pp. 713-727. and G. M. Allen. 1903. The heredity of albinism. Proc. Am. Soc. Arts and Sci., 1, 38, No. 21, p. 603. and H. D. Fish. 1915. The black-and-tan rabbit and the significance of multiple allelomorphs. Amer. Nat., 49, pp. 88-96. and A. Forbes. 1906. Heredity of hair-length in guinea-pigs and its bearing on the theory of pure gametes. Carnegie Inst. Wash. Pub. No. 49, pp. 1-14. and C. C. Little. 1909. The peculiar inheritance of pink eyes among colored mice. Science, n. s., 30, pp. 313-314. and J. C. Phillips. 1914. Piebald rats and selection. Carnegie Inst. Wash. Pub. No. 195, 56 pp., 3 pis. and Walter, Mullenix, and Cobb. 1909. Studies of inheritance in rabbits. Carnegie Inst. Wash. Pub. No. 114, 68 pp., 4 pis. and S. WbiGHT. L915. Two color mutations of rats which show partial coupling. Science, n. is., 42, pp. 193-195. Cole, L. J. 191 1. Studies on inheritance in pigeons. 1. Hereditary relatione of the principal colors. Agri. Exp. Sta., Rhode Island State College, Bull. 158. 188 BIBLIOGRAPHY. 189 Cuenot, L. 1902. La loi de Mendel et I'h6redit6 de la pigmentation chez les souris. Arch. Zool. Exp. et Gen. (3), 10, Notes et revue, p. 27. 1903. 2me note. Ibid. (4), 1, Notes et revue, pp. 33-41. 1904. 3me note. Ibid. (4), 2, Notes et revue, pp. 45-56. 1905. 4me note. Ibid. (4), 3, Notes et revue, pp. 123-132. 1907. 5me note. Ibid. (4), 5, Notes et revue, p. 1. 1908. 6me note. Ibid. (4), 6, Notes et revue, pp. 7-15. 1911. 7me note. Ibid. (4), 8, Notes et revue, pp. 40-56. Darbishire, A. D. 1902. Note on the result of crossing Japanese waltzing mice with European albino races. Biometrika, 2, pp. 101, 165, 282; 3, p. 1. Davenport, C. B., and G. C. Davenport. 1907. Heredity of eye-color in man. Science, n. s., 26, p. 589. 1909. Heredity of hair-color in man. Amer. Nat., 43, p. 193. 1910. Heredity of skin-pigmentation in man. Amer. Nat., 44, p. 641. Detlefsen, J. A. 1914. Genetic studies on a cavy species cross. Carnegie Inst. Wash. Pub. No. 205, 134 pp., 10 pis. Dexter, J. S. 1914. The analysis of a case of continuous variation in Drosophila by a study of its linkage relations. Amer. Nat., 48, pp. 712-758. Durham, F. M. 1904. On the presence of tyrosinases in the skins of some pigmented vertebrates. Proc. Roy. Soc. London, 74, pp. 310-313. 1907. Note on melanins. Jour. Phys., 35, pp. 47-48. 1911. Further experiments on the inheritance of coat color in mice. Jour. Genet., 1, pp. 159-178. East, E. M. 1910. A Mendelian interpretation of variation that is apparently continuous. Amer. Nat., 44, pp. 65-82. and R. A. Emerson. 1913. The inheritance of quantitative characters in maize. Neb. Agri. Exp. Sta. Research Bull., No. 2, 120 pp., 21 figs. Goodale, H. D., and T. H. Morgan. 1913. Heredity of tricolor in guinea-pigs. Amer. Nat., 47, p. 321. GORTNER, R. A. 1910. Spiegler's "white melanin" as related to dominant or recessive white. Amer. Nat., 44, p. 497. 1911. Studies on melanin. IV. The origin of the pigment and color pattern in the elytra of the Colorado potato beetle. Amer. Nat., 45, p. 743. Groth, B. H. A. (Joint author with Halstead and others.) 1913. Experiments with peppers. Report Bot. Dept. N. J. Agri. Col. Exp. Sta., pp. 549-588, illustr. Hagedoorn, A. L. 1912. On tricolor coat in dogs and guinea-pigs. Amer. Nat., 46, p. 682. Hoge, M. A. 1915. The influence of temperature on the development of a Mendelian character. Jour. Exp. Zool., 18, pp. 241-290. Holmes, S. J., and H. M. Loomis. 1910. Eye color and hair color in man. Biol. Bull., 18, p. 50. Hooker, D. 1915. R61es of nucleus and cytoplasm in melanin elaboration. Anat. Rec, 9, p. 393. Jennings, H. S. 1909. Heredity and variation in the simplest organisms. Amer. Nat., 43, pp. 321-337. 1910. Experimental evidence on the effectiveness of selection. Amer. Nat., 44, pp. 136-145. Keeble, F., and E. F. Armstrong. 1912. The role of oxydases in the formation of the anthocyan pigments of plants. Jour. Genet., 2, pp. 277-306. 190 INHERITANCE IN RATS. I.ITTLE, C. C. I'.Ul. The "dilute" forms of yellow mice. Science, n. a., 33, pp. 896-897. l'.M l/». The influence of heredity and of environment in determining the coat colors in mice. Science, n. s., 34, p. 563. 1 « 1 1 3 . Experimental studies of the inheritance of color in mice. Carnegie Inst. w aah. Pub. No. 179, pp. 11-102, 5 pis. 1914. Dominant and recessive spotting in mice. Amer. Nat., 48, pp. 74-82. 1914a. Coat color in pointer dogs. Jour. Her., 5, pp. 244-248. M \cCurdy, H., and W. E. Castle. 1907. Selection and cross-breeding in relation to the inheritance of coat-pigments and coat-patterns in rats and guinea-pigs. Carnegie Inst. Wash. Pub. No. 70, 50 pp., 2 pis. M \< Dowell, E. C. 1914. Size inheritance in rabbits. Carnegie Inst. Wash. Pub. No. 196, 55 pp. Morgan, T. H. 1909. Breeding experiments with rats. Amer. Nat., 43, pp. 182-185. 1911. Moulting and change of color of coat in mice. Science, n. s., 34, p. 918. 1914. Heredity and sex. Columbia University Press. Nehhing, A. 1889. Ueber die Herkunft des haus-Meerschweinchen. Sitzungsb. der Naturf. Gesell. zu Berlin, pp. 1-4, 4 figs. 1893. Ueber Kreuzungen von Cavia aperia und Cavia cobaya. Sitzungsb. der Naturf. Gesell. zu Berlin, No. 10, pp. 247-252. 1894. Kreuzungen von zahmen und wilden Meerschweinchen. Zool. Gart., 35, pp. 1-6, 39-43, 74-78. Nilsson-Ehle, H. 1909. Kreuzungsuntersuchungen an Hafer und Weizen. Lunds Univer. Arsskrift. Onslow, H. 1915. A contribution to our knowledge of the chemistry of coat colors in animals and of dominant and recessive whiteness. Proc. Roy. Soc, B-89, pp. 36-58. Pearson, K., E. Nettleship, and C. H. Usher. 1913. A monograph on albinism in man. Parts I and IV. Dulau and Co., London. Phillips, J. C. 1912. Size inheritance in ducks. Jour. Exp. Zool, 12, pp. 369-380. 1914. A further study of size inheritance in ducks with observations on the sex ratio of hybrid birds. Ibid., 16, pp. 131-148. PUNNETT, R. C. 1912. Inheritance of coat-color in rabbits. Jour, of Genet., 2, p. 221. Riddle, O. 1909. Our knowledge of melanin color formation and its bearing on the Mendelian description. Biol. Bull., 16, pp. 316-351. Ridgway, R. 1912. Color standards and nomenclature. Wash., iv 4- 44 pp., 53 pis. BoUiAB, I. B. J. 1909. Inheritance of color and of supernumerary mammae in guinea-pigs, with a note on the occurrence of a dwarf form. Reports to the Evol. Com. of the Roy. Soc. London, Report 5, pp. 51-79, 1 fig., 1 pi. Stirtevant, A. H. 1913. The Himalayan rabbit case, with some considerations on multiple allelomorphs. Thomas, O. 1901. On mammals obtained by Mr. Alphonse Roberts on the Rio Jordan, S. A., Minas Geraes. Ann. and Mag. Nat. Hist., London, 8, No. 49, pp. 526-539. Troiessart, E. L. 1904. Catalogus Mammalium. Berlin. Wactbboubb, ('.. EL 184s. A natural history of the mammalia. H. Bailliere, n, 500 pp., 21 pis. W II.DKU, II H 1909. H ist ory of the human bodv. 573 pp. W RIGHT, S. 1915 The albino ^i-ies of allelomorphs in guinea-pigs. Amer. Nat., 49, pp. 140-14S. EXPLANATION OF PLATES. PLATE 1. Colored photographs of the fur of guinea-pigs, showing grades of dilution due to different combinations of the allelomorphs of albinism. The skin of each animal from which fur is shown was opened along the median ventral line and a compltte section across the middle of the body is shown in skins 1, 6, 9, 10, and 11. But in the other skins 2, 3, 4, 5, 7, and 8, only a section extending from the mid-dorsal to the mid-ventral line is shown. Figures 1 to 4 show agouti fur (AAEE) ; 5 to 8, non-agouti fur (aaEE) ; and 9 to 11 show fur in which the extension factor is wanting (ee). The uppermost row of skins are intense pigmented, all others are dilute of intensities, diminishing toward the bottom of the plate. Factors A and E are written as homozygous, though this ia not known in all cases. Fig. 1. Black-red agouti C C AAEE 2. Dark sepia-yellow agouti Cd Cd AAEE 3. Dark sepia-cream agouti Cd Cr AAEE 4. Light sepia-cream agouti Cd Ca AAEE 5. Black C C aaEE 6. Dark sepiai Cd Cd aaEE 7. Dark sepia2 Cd Cr aaEE 8. Light sepia6 Cd Ca aaEE 9. Red C C aaee 10. Yellow4 Cd Cd aaee 1 1 . Creanio Cd Ca aaee (Cd Cr similar) PLATE 2. Photographs of guinea-pig skins, showing further grades of reduction in color due to albin- ism and its allelomorphs. Sections of skin extending entirely across the body are shown in all cases. The arrangement is similar to that of plate 1. Fig. 12. Dark sepia-white agouti (red-eyed) Cr Cr AAEE 13. Light sepia-white agouti (red-eyed) Cr Ca AAEE 14. Albino (known to transmit agouti) Ca Ca AAEE 15. Dark sepia (red-eyed) Cr Cr aaEE 16. Light sepia (red-eyed) Cr Ca aaEE 17. Albino (transmits only non-agouti) Ca Ca aaEE 18. White (red-eyed) Cr Cr aaee (Cr Ca similar) 19. Albino (from yellow stock) Ca Ca aaee PLATE 3. Colored photographs of the skins of guinea-pigs. Fig. 20. A half -grown guinea-pig of race C; color, pale cream. The eyes were bro wn-pigmented . 21. An Fi male hybrid whose mother was an albino of race B and whose father was a pure cutleri. Compare figures 23 and 34. 22. An Fi male hybrid whose mother was a brown-eyed cream animal of race C and whose father was a pure cutleri. Compare figures 20 and 23. 23. A pure cutleri male. 24. A pure cutleri female. PLATE 4. Colored photographs of the skins of F2 hybrids produced by crossing brown-eyed cream guinea-pigs with Cavia cutleri. Compare figures 20, 22, and 23. Fig. 25. Golden agouti. 26. Pale black (sepia). 27. Brown or chocolate. 28. Cinnamon. 29. Yellow. 30. Albino. 191 192 EXPLANATION OF PLATES. PLATE 5. Colored photographs of skins showing new color varieties of guinea-pigs. I i'.. 31. Silver cinnamon or red-eyed cinnamon. 32. Rcnl-and-pink-cyed black spotted with white. 33. Pink-eyed golden agouti spotted with red and with white, hence a "tri- color." M. Albino with sooty fur and black pigmented extremities, similar to race B. PLATE 6. 1 . mure of hybrid guinea-pigs and of their parent races, natural size, to show extent of variation. The longest and the shortest femur in each group of individuals is shown with 3 or 4 others of intermediate length placed between them. In the left half of the plate are shown the femurs of males, and in the right half the femurs of females. Top row, Cavia culleri. Second row, race B guinea-pigs. Third row, Fi hybrids pro- duced by the cross of C. cutleri cf X race B 9 • Fourth row, F2 hybrids from the same cross. PLATE 7. Fig. 35. A scale of grades used in describing the pattern of piebald rats. Rats like the pictures toward the left of the scale are known to fanciers as "hooded" ; the grade at the extreme right would be called "Irish" by fanciers. 36. Skins of a pair of rats and of their 9 young. One parent was an "Irish" rat, the other "hooded." Four of the young are hooded, five are Irish. Hooded is re- cessive to Irish in crosses. The Irish parent in this case was a heterozygote. Note individual variation in each group of young. 37. A typical smooth-coated guinea-pig. 38. A rough-coated guinea-pig, well-rosetted, grade A. 39. A poorly-rosetted rough guinea-pig, grade C. PLATE 1 Variations of intensity of coat pigments, due to albino allelomorphs, in agouti series (1-4), black series (5—8), and yellow series (9—11). PLATE 2 Albinism and its non-yellow allelomorphs in agouti series (12 — 14), Mark series (15—17), and yellow series (18, 19). PLATE 3 20 21 22 23 24 SmjpBB^^ ^^*" Fig. 20, half-grown guinea-pig, race C. Figs. 23, 24, male and female Cavia cutleri, adult. Fig. 22, F\ hybrid, race C x Cavia cutleri, adult. Fig. 21, F1 hybrid, race B (Plate 5, Fig. 34) x Cavia cutleri, adult. PLATE 4 25 26 27 F2 hybrids, race C x Cavia cutleri. Fig. 25, agouti; 26, black; 27, chocolate; 28, cinnamon; 29, yellow; 30, albino. PLATE 5 31 32 34 Some new guinea-pig color varieties. Fig. 31, red-eyed cinnamon; 32, red-and-pink-eyed black spotted with white; 33, pink-eyed golden agouti spotted with red and with white; 34, albino with sooty fur and black pigmented extremities, similar to race B. PLATE 6 Femurs of Cavia cutleri (male and female), of race B, and of their FL and F^ hybrids, showing complete range of variation in each. Natural size. PLATE 7 — 1 + 1 4-3 4- 36 35 37 38 39 Fig. 35, a scale of grades for piebald rats, n*"'*^^^^^^ young. Fig. 37, a smooth guinea-pig. Fig. 38, a well-rosettea rougn gu caps-s A. Fig. 39, a poorly rosetted rough guinea-pig, grade L-. QH 431 C3925 Castle, William Ernest Studies of inheritance BioMed. PLEASE DO NOT REMOVE CARDS OR SLIPS FROM THIS POCKET UNIVERSITY OF TORONTO LIBRARY ■