Columtiia Simbergitp Vb. — Effect of Various Influences on the Nerve Impulse, 117. — The Fatigue of Nerve Fibers, US. — The Metabolism of the Nerve Fiber during Functional Activity, 120. — Theories of the Nerve Impulse. 121. — Qualitative Differences in Nerve Impulses, 121. — Doctrine of Specific Nerve Energies, 124. — Nutritive Relations of Nerve Fibers and Nerve Cells. 12.">. — Nerve Degen- eration and Regeneration, 127. — Degenerative Changes in the Central End of the Neuron. 129 SECTION II. THE PHYSIOLOGY OF THE CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. Chapter VI.- Structure and General Properties of the Nerve Cell 131 The Neuron Doctrine, 131. — The Varieties of Neurons, 133.— Internal St rueture of the Nerve Cell, 136. — General Physiology of the Nerve Cell, 137. — Sum- mation i.f Stimuli in Nerve Cells, 139. — Response of the Nerve Cell to Varying Rates of Stimulation, 140. — The Refractory Period of the Nerve Cell, 141. Chapter VII. — Reflex Actions 1 12 Definition and Historical, 142.— The Reflex Are, 142.— The Reflex Frog, 144.— Spinal Reflex Movements, H4. 1 heory of Co-ordinated Reflexes, 140. — Spinal Reflexes in Mammal-, [47.— Dependence of Co-ordinated Reflexes upon the Excitation of the Sensory Endings, 149. — Reflex Time, 148. — Inhibition of Reflexes, 149. — Influence of the Condition of the Cord on its Reflex Act ivit ies, 151.— Reflexes from other Parts of the Nervous System, 151.— Reflexes Through Peripheral Ganglia, Axon Reflexes, 152. — The Tonic Activity of t he Spinal Cord, 154.- Effects of the Removal of the Spinal Cord, 155. — Knee-jerk, 150. — Reinforcement of the Knee-jerk, 156. — Is the Knee-jerk a Reflex Act? 158. — Conditions Influencing the Extent of the Knee-jerk. 159. — The Knee- jerk and Spinal Reflexes as Diagnostic Signs, 160. — Other Spinal Reflexes, 101. Chapter YIH. — The Spinal Cord as a Path of Conduction 1(53 Arrangement and Classification of the Nerve Cells in the Cord, 103. — General Relations Of the Gray and White Matter in the Cord. 165. — The Methods of Determining the Tracts of the Cord, 165. — General Classification of the Tracts of the Cord, 100.— The Names and I. ocat ions of t he Long Tracts, 168. — The [termination in the Cord of the libers of the Posterior Root, 169.— Ascend- ing or Afferent Paths in the Posterior Funiculi, 170. — Ascending or Afferent Paths in the Lateral Funiculi, 173. — The Spinal Paths for the Cutaneous Senses 'Touch. Pain, Temperature), 175. The Homolateral or Contralateral Conduction of the Cutaneous Impulses, 170. — The Descending or Efferent Paths in the Anf rolateral Columns (Pyramidal System), 177. — Less Well- known Tracts in the Cord, 179. Chapter IX. The Gi neral Physiology of the Cerebrum and Its Motor Fi motions 182 The Histology of the Cortex, 183, 'l he Classification of the Systems of libers in the Cerebrum (Projection, Association, and Commissural), L84. Physio- logical Deductions from the Histology of the Cortex, L86.— Extirpation of the brum, 188. Localization of Functions in the Cerebrum, Historical, 190. Motoi Vrea of the Cortex, 193. Differences in Paralysis from Injury to the Spinal Neuron and the Pyramidal Neuron, 195. Voluntary Motor Path* Othei than the Pyramidal Tract, 195. The Crossed Control of the Muscle and Bilateral Motoi Representation in the Cortex, 196. Arc the Motoi Vrea I clusi elj Motoi ? 197. Chapter X. The Sense Areas and the Association Areas in the L99 Tl,. Vrea, 200, ["heCourseof the Lemniscus, 201, TheCenterfor Vision, 203 lb tol igical Evidence of the Coui e of the Optic Fibers, 204. The Decu ation in the Chia ma, 206. The Projection of the Retina on the 206 The Function of the Lower Visual Centers, 209. 4 he Auditory ('enter, 209. Coui e of the Cochlear Nerve, 211, ["he Phj io logical .-. ol the Lowei Vuditorj Centers, 212. Motoi Responses from tl,.- Audi'. Cor! 113 Th< Olfactory Center, 213. The Olfactorj Bulb and it- Connection . 2U The Cortical Centei for Smell, 215. The Cortical Centei foi Taste, 215. Vpha ia, 215 Sen or} Vpha ia, 218, The \ ociation An ubdivi ion ol the \ ociation Vrea . 221 pie D< elnpment of the Cortical Vrea !22 Histological Differentiation in Coi tical stiu.iuie, 225, Phj ioloin ol the Corpus Callosum, 220. Physiology of the Corpora Striata and fhafami, 227. TABLE OF CONTENTS. 9 PAGE Chapter XL — The Functions of the Cerebellum, the Pons, and the Medulla 229 Anatomical Structure and Relations of the Cerebellum, 229. — General State- ment of Theories Regarding the Cerebellum, 233. — Experiments upon Ablation of the Cerebellum, 234. — Interpretation of the Experimental and Clinical Results, 235. — Conclusions as to the General Functions of the Cerebellum, 237. — The Psychical Functions of the Cerebellum, 239. — Localization of Func- tion in the Cerebellum, 239. — The Functions of the Medulla Oblongata, 240. — The Nuclei of Origin and the Functions of the Cranial Nerves, 241. Chapter XII. — The Sympathetic or Autonomic Nervous System.. 246 General Statements, 246. — Autonomic Nervous System, 247. — The Use of the Nicotin Method, 248. — General Course of the Autonomic Fibers Arising from the Cord, 248. — General Course of the Fibers Arising from the Brain, 249. — -General Course of the Fibers Arising from the Sacral Cord, 251. — Normal Mode of Stimulation of Autonomic Nerve Fibers, 251. Chapter XIII. — The Physiology of Sleep 253 General Statements, 253. — Physiological Relations during Sleep, 253. — The Intensity of Sleep, 254. — Changes in the Circulation during Sleep, 256. — Effect of Sensory Stimulation, 259. — Theories of Sleep, 260. — Hypnotic Sleep, 263. SECTION III. THE SPECIAL SENSES. Chapter XIV. — Classification of the Senses and General State- ments 264 Classification of the Senses, 264. — The Doctrine of Specific Nerve Energies, 266.— The Weber-Fechner Psychophysical Law, 268. Chapter XV. — Cutaneous and Internal Sensations 271 General Classification, 271. — The Punctiform Distribution of the Cutaneous Senses, 272. — Specific Nerve Energies of the Cutaneous Nerves, 274. — The Temperature Senses, 275. — The Sense of Pressure, 276. — The Threshold Stimu- lus and the Localizing Power, 276. — The Pain Sense, 279. — Localization or Projection of Pain Sensations, 280. — Reflected or Misreferred Pains, 280. — Muscular or Deep Sensibility, 2S0. — The Qualitv of the Muscular Sensibility, 282.— Sensations of Hunger and Thirst, 283.— The Sense of Thirst, 285. Chapter XVI. — Sensations of Taste and Smell 286 The Nerves of Taste, 286. — The End-organ of the Taste Fibers, 288.— Classi- fication of Taste Sensations, 288. — Distribution and Specific Energy of the Fundamental Taste Sensations, 289. — Method of Sapid Stimulation, 290. — The Threshold Stimulus for Taste, 291.— The Olfactory Organ, 291.— The Mechanism of Smelling, 292. — Nature of the Olfactory Stimulus, 293.— The Qualities of the Olfactory Sensations, 293. — Fatigue of the Olfactory Apparatus, 295. — Delicacy of the Olfactory Sense, 295. — Conflict of Olfactory Sensations, 297. — Olfactory Associations, 297. Chapter XVII. — The Eye as an Optical Instrument. Dioptrics of the Eye 298 Formation of an Image by a Biconvex Lens, 298. — Formation of an Image in the Eye, 301. — The Inversion of the Image on the Retina, 303. — The Size of the Retinal Image, 304. — Accommodation of the Eye, 305. — Limit of the Power of Accommodation and Near Point of Distinct Vision, 308. — Far Point of Distinct Vision, 309. — The Refractive Power of the Surfaces in the Eye, 309. — Optical Defects of the Normal Eye, 310. — Spherical Aberration, 311. — Abnormalities in the Refraction of the Eye, Myopia, 311. — Hypermetropia, 312. — Presbyopia, 313. — Astigmatism, 314. — Innervation and Control of the Ciliary Muscle and the Muscles of the Iris, 316. — The Accommodation Reflex and the Light Reflex, 318. — Action of Drugs upon the Iris, 320. — The Antagonism of the Sphincter and Dilator Muscles of the Iris, 321. — Intraocular Pressure, 322. — The Ophthalmoscope, 323. — Retinoscope, 325. — Ophthalmometer, 326. Chapter XVI 1 1. — The Properties of the Retina. Visual Stimuli and Visual Sensations 328 The Portion of the Retina Stimulated by Light, 328. — The Action Current Caused by Stimulation of the Retina, 329. — The Visual Purple, Rhodopsin, 330. — Extent of the Visual Field, Perimetry, 332. — Central and Peripheral Fields of Vision, 333. — Visual Acuity, 335. — Relation Between Stimulus and Sensation, Threshold Stimulus, 337. — The Light Adapted and the Dark Adapted Eye, 338. — Luminosity or Brightness, 339. — Qualities of Visual 10 TABLE OF CONTENTS. PAGH Sensations, 340.— The Achromatic Series. 341. — The Chromatic Series, 311. — Color Saturation and Color Fusion, 341. — The Fundamental Colors, 342. — The Complementary Color-, 343. — After Images, Positive and Negative, 343. — Color Contrasts, 344. — Color Blindness, 345. — Dichromatic Vision, 346. — Tests for Color Blindness, 347. — Monochromatic Vision, 34S. — Distribution of Color Sense in the Retina. 348. — Functions of the Rods and Cones, 349. — Theories of Color Vision, 352. — Entoptic Phenomena, 357. — Shadows of Corpuscles and Blood-vessels, 357. — Shadows from Lens and Vitreous Humor, 358. Chapter XLX.— Binocular Vision 359 Movements of the Eyeballs, 359. — Co-ordination of the Eye Muscles, Muscular Insufficiency and Strabismus, 361. — The Binocular Field of Vision, 362. idmg or Identical Points, 362. — Physiological Diplopia, 364. — The Horopter, 365. — Suppression of Visual Images, 365. — Struggle of the Visual Fields, :'.tit>. — Judgments of Solidity, 306. — Monocular Perspective, 367. — Binocular Perspective, 368. — Stereoscopic Vision, 369. — Explanation of Binoc- ular Perspective, 371. — Judgments of Distance and Size, 371. — Optical Decep- tion-, 372. Chapter XX.— The Eak as an Organ for Sound Sensations 375 The Pinna or Auricle, 376. — The Tympanic Membrane, 376. — The Ear Bones, 377. — Mode of Action of the Ear Bones, 378. — Muscles of the Middle Ear, 380. — The Eustachian Tube, 381. — Projection of the Auditory Sensations. 382 Sensory Epithelium of the Cochlea, 382. — Nature and Action of the Sound Waves, 3S3. — Classification and Properties of Musical Sounds, 384. — Upper Harmonics or Overtones. 386. — Sympathetic Vibrations and Resonance, 388. — Functions of the Cochlea, 388. — Sensations of Harmony and Discord, 391. —Limits of Hearing, 392. Chapter XXL— Functions of the Semicircular Canals and the Vestibule 394 Position and Structure of the Semicircular Canals, 394. — Flouren's Experi- ments upon the Semicircular Canals, 395. — Temporary and Permanent Effects of the Operations, 396. — Effect of Direct Stimulation of the Canals, 397. — Kffcct of Section of the Ampullary or the Acoustic Nerve, 397. — Is the Effect of Section of the Canals Due to Stimulation? 397. — Theories of the Functions of the Semicircular Canals, 398. — Summary of the Views upon the Function of the Semicircular Canals, 400. — Functions of the Utriculus and Sacculus, 402. SECTION IV. BLOOD AND LYMPH. Chapter XXII. — General Properties of Blood. Physiology OP the Corpuscles 404 Histological Structure of Blood, 404.— Reaction of the Blood, 405. — Specific Gravity of the Blood, 407.— The lied Corpuscles, 408. Condition of the Hemoglobin in the Corpuscles, 408. Hemolysis, 409. Hemolysis Due to \aiia1ion in I) i.Hilic Pressure, 410.- Hemolysis Due to Action of Hemoly- tic -Nature ami \ ml of Hemoglobin. III. Compounds of Hemo- globin with Oxygen and Other < rases, 416.- The I ion in the I Icmoglobin, I 17. — Crystals of Hemoglobin, 418.— Absorption Spectra of Hemoglobin and Oxy- hemoglobin, 419.- Derivative Compounds ot Hei dobin, 423. origin and Fate of the Red Corpuscles, 125. variations in the Number of Red Corpuscles, 127. Physiology of the Blood Leucocytes, 429. — Variations in Number of the Leucocytes, 431.- Functions of the Leucocytes, 431. — Physiology of the Blood Plates, 432. Chapter XXIII. Chemical Composition op the Blood Plasma; Coagulation; Quantiti of Blood; Regeneration after 1 1 BMORRH \i;i. 434 Compo it ion of tin- I 'la ma and Corpuscles. 134. Proteins of the Blood Plasma, 436. Serum Albumin, 436 Paraglobulm (Serum Globulin), 437. Fibrino- gen, 138. Le Well-known Proteins of the Blood, 440. Coagulation of Blood, MO. Theorie of Clotting, I il'. Preparation of Solutions of Fibrino- gen, 142. Preparation of Thrombin. 443. The factors of Coagulation, ii .: Propertie and Origin of Thrombin, in Summary, I M'>. The Con- vi-i ion of Fibrinogen to Fibrin, 447. whj Bl I Hoc. n,,i Clot Within the Vessels. 44 culai Clotting, 149 Means of Ha tening or of Rel ulaiion. 160, Total Quantity of Blood in the Body, 462. — • ration "i the Blood after Hemorrhage, 453. Blood Transfusion, 155 Chapter XXIV. Composition \m» Formation of Lymph 466 eral S atement 156 Formation, of Lymph, 157. Lymphagpgues of the First CI 169 I I the Second Class, 160, Bummary of tin- Factoi Controlling the Mow of Lymph, i<>~. TABLE OF CONTENTS. 11 SECTION V. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE ORGANS OF CIRCULATION OF THE BLOOD AND LYMPH. PAGE Chapter XXV. — The Velocity and Pressure of the Blood Flow 465 The Circulation as Seen under the Microscope, 465. — The Velocity of the Blood Flow, 466. — Mean Velocity in the Arteries, Veins, and Capillaries, 469. — Cause of the Variations in Velocity, 471. — Variations of Velocity with the Heart Beat or Changes in the Blood-vessels, 471. — Time Necessary for a Com- plete Circulation of the Blood, 472. — The Pressure Relations in the Vascular System, 473. — Methods of Recording Blood-pressure, 473. — Systolic, Dias- tolic, and Mean Arterial Pressure, 479. — Method of Measuring Systolic and Diastolic Pressure in Animals, 479. — Data as to the Mean Pressure in Arteries, Veins, and Capillaries, 481. — Methods of Determining Blood-pressure in the Large Arteries of Man, 485. — Normal Arterial Pressure in Man and Its Varia- tions, 490. — The Method of Determining Venous Pressures and Capillary Pressures in Man, 491. Chapter XXVI. — The Physical Factors Concerned in the Produc- tion op Blood-pressure and Blood-velocity 495 Side Pressure and Velocity Pressure, 495. — The Factors Concerned in Pro- ducing Normal Pressure and Velocity, 498. — General Conditions Influencing Blood-pressure and Blood-velocity, 499. — The Hydrostatic Effect, 500. — Accessory Factors Aiding the Circulation, 502. — The Conditions of Pressure and Velocity in the Pulmonary Circulation, 503. — Variations of Pressure in the Pulmonary Circuit, 503. Chapter XXVII.— The Pulse 505 General Statement, 505. — Velocity of the Pulse Wave, 506. — Form of the Pulse Wave, Sphygmography, 508. — Explanation of the Catacrotic Waves, 510. — Anacrotic Waves, 511. — The Kinds of Pulse in Health and Disease, 512.— Venous Pulse, 513. Chapter XXVIIL— The Heart Beat 518 General Statement, 518. — Musculature of the Auricles and Ventricles, 519. — The Auriculoventricular Bundle, 520. — Contraction Wave of the Heart, 521. — The Electrical Variation, 522. — Change of Form during Systole, 523. — The Apex Beat, 524.— Cardiogram, 525. — Intraventricular Pressure during Sys- tole, 526. — The Volume Curve and the Ventricular Output, 528.— The Heart Sounds, 531. — The Third Heart Sound, 533. — Events occurring during a cardiac cycle, 534. — Time Relations of Systole and Diastole, 535. — Normal Capacity of Ventricle and Work Done by the Heart, 535.— Coronary Cir- culation during the Heart-beat, 537. — Suction-pump Action of the Heart, 539. — Occlusion of the Coronary Vessels, 541. — Fibrillar Contractions of Heart Muscle, 541. Chapter XXIX. — The Cause and the Sequence of the Heart Beat. Properties of the Heart Muscle 543 General Statement, 543. — The Neurogenic Theory of the Heart Beat, 545. — Myogenic Theory, 546. — Automaticity of the Heart, 548. — Action of Calcium, Potassium, and Sodium Ions on the Heart, 549.— Connection of Inorganic Salts with the Causation of the Beat, 550. — Maximal Contractions of the Heart, 552. — Refractory Period of the Heart Beat, 552. — The Compensatory Pause, 554. — Normal Sequence of the Heart Beat, 554. — Tonicity of the Heart Muscle, 558. Chapter XXX. — The Cardiac Nerves and Their Physiological Action 561 Course of the Cardiac Nerves, 561. — Action of the Inhibitory Fibers, 561. — Analysis of the Inhibitory Action, 563. — Effect of Vagus on the Aur cle and the Ventricle, 565. — Escape from Inhibition, 566. — Reflex Inhibition of the Heart Beat, the Cardio-inhibitory Center, 566. — The Tonic Activity of the Cardio-inhibitorv Center, 567. — The Action of Drugs on the Inhibitory Appara- tus, 559. — The Nature of Inhibition, 569. — Course of the Accelerator Fibers, 571. — Action of the Accelerator Fibers, 573. — Tonicity of the Accelerators and Reflex Acceleration, 573. — The Accelerator Center, 575. Chapter XXXI. — The Rate of the Heart Beat and Its Variations under Normal Conditions 576 Variations in Rate with Sex, Size, and Age, 576. — Variations through the Extrinsic Cardiac Nerves, 557. — Variations with Blood-pressure, 577. — With Muscular Exercise, 578. — With the Gases of the Blood, 579. — With Tempera- ture of the Blood, 579. 12 TABLE OF CONTENTS. PAGE Chapter XXXII. — The Vasomotor Nerves and Theih Physiological ACTIVITY 582 Historical, 582.— Methods Used to Determine Vasomotor Action, 583. — The Plethysmograph, oS4. — General Distribution and Course of the Vasoconstrictor Conic Activity of the Vasoconstrictors, 589. — Ihe \ aso- constrictor Center, 589. — Vasoconstrictor Reflexes. Pressor and Depressor Fibers, 591. — Depressor Nerve of the Heart, 594. — Vasoconstrictor Centers in the Spinal Cord, 595. — Rhythmical Activity of the Vasoconstrictor Center, 595. — Course and Distribution of the Dilator Fibers, 596.— General Properties of Vasodilator Fibers, 597. — Vasodilator Cepter and Reflexes, 597.— Vasodila- tation Due to Antidromic Impulses, o'.ui. — Regulation of the Blood-supply by Chemical and Mechanical Stimuli, Gnu. Chapter XXXIII. — Thk Vasomotor Supply of the Different ( Irgans 601 Vasomotors of the Heart, 601. — Vasomotors of the Pulmonary Arteries, 602. — Circulation in the Brain and Its Regulation, 603. — Arterial Supply, tio3. — Venous Supply, 604. — The Meningeal Spaces, 605. — Intracranial Pressure, 0i>7. — Effect of Changes in Arterial Pressure upon the Blood-How through the Brain, 60S. — The Regulation of the Brain Circulation, 610— Vasomotor Nerves ,,f the Head Region, 612. — Of the Trunk and the Limbs, 613. — Of the Abdominal » Organs, 613. -Of the Genital Organs, 014.— Of the Skeletal Muscles, 614.— The Vasomotor Nerves to the Veins, 615. — The Circulation of the Lymph, tilt;. SECTION VI. PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. Chapter XXXIV. — Historical Statement. The Organs of Exter- nal Respiration and the Respiratory Movements 618 Historical, 618. — Anatomy of Organs of Respiration, 622. — Thorax as a Closed Cavity, 622.— Normal Position of the Thorax. 623. Inspiration by ( ontraction of the Diaphragm, 024. — Inspiration by Elevation of the Ribs, 625.— The Muscles of Inspiration, 626.— Muscles of Expiration, 626.— Quiet and Forced piratory Movements, Eupnea and Dsypnea, 0: 7. — Costal and Abdominal types of Respiration, 628.— Accessory Respiratory Movements, 629.— Registration of the Respiratory Movements, 629. Volumes of Air Respired Vital Capacity, Tidal Air, Complemental Air, Supplemental Air, Residual Minimal Air. 631. — Size of the Bronchial Tree, 633.— Artiluial Respira- tion, 633. Chapter XX XV- The Pressure Conditions in the Lungs and Thorax and Their Enfli ence opon the Circulation 635 The [ntrapulmonic Pressure and Its Variation 635. [ntrathroacic Pressure, Lriations of, with Forced and Cnu-ual Respirations, 637. -Origin ..I theN Pn ure in the Thorax. 638. Pneumothorax, o.U) \spuatory Action of the Thorax. 639. Re piratory Waves of Blood-pressure, 640. Chapter XXXVI. The Chemical and Physical Changes i\ the Air wi, ! hi. Blood Caused by Respir \n<>\ b44 1 and Expired \i,. 644. rPhysical Changes in the Expir. ,d Air, 644 [niii i Vction of Expired Vir, 645. Ventilation, 64 flhe Oases of he PI I. 648 The Pre I Ga es, 651. Abeoi p to .n o G Liauids, 651 The Tension of Ga«e« in Solul ,, • The Conchtwn o .„ in the Blood 654 Condition of Oxygen m.theBl I, 655. Con- Stion^fCarWDfoxidin the HI 1.656 Tfie Phy si. a 'I heory ■■•;■;•'•',. Hon, 858 Ga « tl • I ling 6 f I change wea in tn< I . ., -,,-i,, i- Activity of Lung . 661 • ChapterXXXVII [nnbrvation of the Respirators Movements. The i ' iter, 662 Spinal Re piratory Centei 663.( -Automatic tv of the R« piraton Centei 864 Reflex Stimulation of the V®"^!; Vfferenl Relation! of the Vagu to the Center, 866. [he inspirators and [nhibil of the Vagu . 668 Reepiraton Reflexei Fro"1 the i \,, ,. B6fl Voluntary Control of the Re piratorj Nature ol the Re piratory Center, 870 Re piraton I en n the Midbrain 672 Automatic Stimulus to the Ri piratpr; Center, I Re piratoi M ■• • D: a, Hyperpnea, ..,,,,! Apnea 676 Innervation ol the Bronchial Mu culature, <>78. (162 TABLE OF CONTENTS. 13 PAGE Chapter XXXVIII. — The Influence of Various Conditions upon the Respiration 680 Effect of Muscular Work on the Respiratory Movements, 680. — Effect of Variations in the Composition of the Air, 680. — High and Low Barometric Pressures, Mountain Sickness, Caisson Disease, 682. — The Respiratory Quotient and Its Variations, 6S4. — Modified Respiratory Movements, 686. SECTION VII. PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. Chapter XXXIX. — Movements of the Alimentary Canal 689 Mastication, 689. — Deglutition, 689. — Nervous Control of Deglutition, 693. — Anatomy of the Stomach, 694. — Musculature of the Stomach, 695. — Move- ments of the Stomach, 696. — Effect of the Nerves on the Movements of the Stomach, 699. — Movements of the Intestines, 700. — Peristaltic and Pendular Movements of the Intestines, 700. — Nervous Control of the Intestinal Move- ments, 703. — Effect of Various Conditions on the Intestinal Movements, 704. — Movements of the Large Intestines, 705. — Defecation, 706. — Vomiting, 709. — Nervous Mechanism of Vomiting, 710. Chapter XL. — General Consideration of the Composition of the Food and the Action of Enzymes 712 Foods and Foodstuffs, 712. — Accessory Articles of Diet, 714. — Enzymes, Historical, 715. — Reversible Reactions, 717. — Specificity of Enzymes, 719. — Definition and Classification of Enzymes, 720. — General Properties of Enzymes, 721. — Partial List of Enzymes, 722. — Chemical Composition of the Enzymes, 723. Chapter XLI. — The Salivary Glands and Their Digestive Action 724 Anatomy of the Salivary Glands, 724. — Histological Structure, 726. — Com- position of the Secretion, 727. — The Secretory Nerves, 728. — Trophic and Secretory Nerve Fibers, 730. — Histological Changes during Activity, 734. — Action of Drugs upon the Secretory Nerves, 734. — Paralytic Secretion, 735. — Normal Mechanism of Salivary Secretion, 736. — Electrical Changes in Glands, 737. — Digestive Action of Saliva, Ptyalin, 737. — Conditions Influencing the Action of Ptyalin, 738. — Functions of the Saliva, 739. Chapter XLIL— Digestion and Absorption in the Stomach 740 Structure of the Gastric Glands, 740. — Histological Changes during Secretion, 741. — Method of Obtaining the Gastric Secretion and Its Normal Composition, 742. — The Acid of Gastric Juice, 744. — Origin of the HC1, 745. — Secretory Nerves of the Gastric Glands, 746. — Normal Mechanism of the Secretion of the Gastric Juice, 747. — Nature and Properties of Pepsin, 749. — Artificial Gastric Juice, 751. — Pepsin-hydrochloric Digestion, 751. — The Rennin En- zyme, 753. — Digestive Changes in the Stomach, 755. — Absorption in the Stomach, 756. Chapter XLlII. — Digestion and Absorption in the Intestines. . . 759 Structure of the Pancreas, 759. — Composition of the Secretion, 760. — Secre- tory Nerve Fibers to the Pancreas, 760. — The Curve of Secretion, 761. — Nor- mal Mechanism of Pancreatic Secretion, 762. — Secretin, 763. — Enterokinase, 763. — Digestive Action of Pancreatic Juice, 764. — Significance of Tryptic Diges- tion, 766. — Action of the Diastatic Enzyme (Amylase), 768. — Action of the Lipolytic Enzyme (Lipase, Steapsin), 768. — The Intestinal Secretion (Succus Entericus), 770.— Absorption in the Small Intestine, 771. — Absorption of Carbohydrates, 773. — Absorption of Fats, 774. — Absorption of Proteins, 775.— Digestion and Absorption in the Large Intestine. 777. — Bacterial Action in the Small Intestine, 778. — Bacterial Action in the Large Intestine, 779. — Physiological Importance of Intestinal Putrefaction, 779. — Composition of the Feces, 780. Chapter XLIV. — Physiology of the Liver and Spleen 782 Structure of the Liver, 782.— Composition of Bile, 783. — The Bile Pigments, 784. — The Bile Acids, 785. — Cholesterin, 787. — Lecithin. Fats, and Nucleo- albumins, 787. — Secretion of the Bile, 788. — Ejection of the Bile — Function of the Gall-bladder, 789. — Occlusion of the Bile-ducts, 791. — Physiological Importance of Bile, 791. — Occurrence of Glycogen, 792. — Origin of Glycogen, 793. — Function of Glycogen, Glycogenic Theory. 795. — Glycogen in the Muscles and Other Tissue, 797. — Conditions Affecting the Supply of Glycogen, 798. — Formation of Urea in the Liver, 798. — Physiology of the Spleen, 799. 14 TABLE OF CONTEXTS. PAGE Chapter XLY. — The Kidney and Skin as Excretory Organs 802 Structure of ihe Kidney, SU2. — The Secretion of Urine, 803. — Function of the Glomerulus, 805.— Function of the Convoluted Tubule, 807. — Action of Diuretics. 809. — The Blood-flow Through the Kidneys, 809. — The Composi- tion of Urine, 812. — The Nitrogenous Excreta in the Urine, 813.- — Origin and Significance of Urea, 814. — Origin and Significance of the Turin Bodies ^'ric Acid, Xanthin. Hypoxanthin), 817. — Orighi and Significance of the Creatinin and Creatin, 820.- Hippuric Acid, 821. — The Conjugated Sulphates and the Sulphur Excretion, S22. — Secretion of the Water and Inorganic Salts, 823. — Micturition. 824.— Contractions of the Bladder, S25. — Nervous Mechanism of Micturition. 827. — Excretory Functions of the Skin, 828. — Composition of Sweat, 829. — Secretory Fibers of Sweat (Hands. 830. — Sweat Centers, 832. — Sebaceous Secretion, S32. — Excretion of Carbon Dioxid through the Skin, 833. Chapter XLYI. — Secretion of the Ductless Glands — Internal Secretion 834 Internal Secretion of Liver, 835. — Internal Secretion of the Thyroid Tissues, 835. — Extirpation of Thyroids and Parathyroids, 836. — Function of the Para- thyroids, 836. — Function of the Thyroid. 838. — Cyon's View of Function of Thyroid, 840. — Function of Thymus, S40. — Structure and Properties of Adrenal Bodies, 841. — General Function of Adrenals, 843. — Pituitary Body, 844. — Internal Secretion of Testis and Ovary, 847. — Internal Secretion of Pancreas, 849. — Internal Secretion of Kidney, 851. SECTION VIII. NUTRITION AND HEAT PRODUCTION AND REGULATION. Chapter XLVII. — General Methods. History of the Protein Food 852 General Statement, 852.— Nitrogen Equilibrium, 852. — Carbon Equilibrium and Body Equilibrium, 854. — Balance Experiments, 854. — Respiration Cham- ber, 855. — Effect of Non-protein Food on Nitrogen Equilibrium, 855. — Nutritive History of the Protein Food, 856. — Tissue Protein and Circulating Protein, 856. — Amount of Protein Necessary in Normal Nutrition, 858. — Specific Character of Protein Metabolism, 861. — Specific Dynamic Action of Proteins, 864. — Nutritive Value of Albuminoids, 865. Chapter XLVIII. — Nutritive History of Carbohydrates and Fats 868 The Carbohydrate Supply of the Body, SOS. — Intermediary Metabolism of the Carbohydrate in the Body, 869. — Regulation of the Sugar Supply of the Body, 870.— Functions of the Carbohydrate Fond. 873. — Nutritive Value of Fats, 874.— Fate of Fats in the Tissues, 875.— Origin of Body Fat, 877.— Origin of Body Fat from Food Fat, 878. — Origin of Body Fat from Carbohydrates, 878. Source of 1 -at in Ordinary Diets, 879. Cause of the Formation of Fat, Obesity, 879.— General Functions of Fat, 880. Chapter KLIX. Ni trh i\ e Yam b of the Inorganic Salts ind the Accessory Articles of Diet 881 The Inorganic Salts of the Body. 881.— Effect of Ash-free and Ash-poor Diets, 882.- Bpecial Importance of Sodium Chlorid, Calcium, and Iron Sails, 882. — The Condiment , Flavors, and Stimulants, 885.— Physiological Effects of Alcohol, 886. Chapter I.. Effect oi Muscular Work i.nd Temperature on Body Metabolism; Beao Energi oi Foods; Dietetics. 890 The Effect of Muscular Worl ,890 Effect of Sleep, 892.- Effect of Variations remperature, 893. Effect of Starvation, 893.- The Potential Energj of I ood, 896 Di< tetic , 898. Chapter i.i The Production oi Hi m m the Body; Its Measure- d Regulation; Bodi Temperature; Calorimetryj Physiological Oxidations ■,"'1 lb torical Account of Theorie of Animal Heat, 904. Body Temperature in Man, 906 Cal 907 Heat Regulation, 912. Regulation <y Welby, and Engelmann, "Archiv f ur die gesammte Physiologic, " 18, L. THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 21 much in smoothness and delicacy by this arrangement. The physical advantages of the extensibility and elasticity of mus- cular tissue are evident not only in the contractions of our volun- tary muscles, but, as we shall see, in a striking way also in the circulation, in which the force of the heart beat is stored and economically distributed by the elastic tension of the distended arteries. The extensibility of muscular tissue has been studied in comparison with the extensibility of dead elastic bodies. With regard to the latter it is known that the strain that the body undergoes is proportional, within the limits of elasticity, to the stress put upon it. If, for instance, weights are attached to a rubber band suspended at one end, the amount of extension of the band will be directly proportional to the weights used. If the extensions are measured the relationship may be represented as shown in the accom- panying figure, the equal increments in weight being indicated by laying off equal distances on the abscissa, and the resulting extensions by the height of the orclinates dropped from each point. If the ends of the orclinates are joined, the result is a straight line. When a similar experiment is made with a living muscle it is found that the extension is not proportional to the weight used. The amount of ex- tension is greatest in the beginning and decreases proportionately with new increments of weight. If the results of such an experiment are plotted, as above, representing the equal increments of weight by equal distances along the abscissa and the resulting extensions by ordi- nates dropped from these points, then upon joining the ends of the orclinates we obtain a curve concave to the abscissa. At first the muscle shows a relatively large extension, but the effect becomes less and less with each new increment of weight, the curve at the end approaching slowly to a horizontal. If the weight is increased until it is sufficient to overcome the elasticity of the muscle the curve is altered — it becomes convex to the abscissa, or, in other words, the amount of extension increases Fig. 5. — Curve given by Marey to show the effect upon the extension of muscle caused by increasing the load regularly to the point of rupture : From o to a the extension of the muscle decreases as the weight increases, giving a curve concave to the abscissa, ox ; at a the limit of elasticity is passed and the muscle lengthens by in- creasing extensions for equal increments ; at x rupture (750 gms. for frog's gastrocnemius). 22 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. with increasing increments of weight up to the point of rupture, as shown in the accompanying curve* (Fig. 5). Hayeraftf calls attention to the fact that under normal conditions the physio- logical extension of the frog's muscles in the body is equal to that produced by a weight of 10 to 15 gms., and that when the excised muscle is extended by weights below this limit it follows the law of dead elastic bodies, giving equal extensions for equal increments of weight. It is only after passing this limit that the law stated above holds good. It should be added also that the amount of deformation exhibited by a muscle or other living tissue placed under a stress varies with the time that the stress is allowed to act. The muscle is composed of viscous material, and yields slowly to the force acting upon it. In experiments of this kind, therefore, the weights should be allowed to act for equal intervals of time. It has been shown that the extensibility of a muscle is greater in the contracted than in the resting state. The curve of extension described above for skeletal muscle holds also for so-called plain muscle. This latter tissue forms a portion of the walls of the various viscera, the stomach, bladder, uterus, blood-vessels, etc., and the facts shown by the above curve enter frequently into the explanation of the physical phenomena exhibited by the viscera. For instance, it follows from this curve that the force of the heart beat will cause less expansion in an artery already distended by a high blood-pressure than in one in which the blood-pressure is lower. The Irritability and Contractility of Muscle. — Under normal conditions in the body a muscle is made to contract by a stimulus received from the central nervous system through its motor nerve. l\' the latter is severed the muscle is paralyzed. We owe to Haller, the great physiologist of the eighteenth century, the proof that a muscle thus isolated can still be made to contract by an artificial stimulus — e. g., an electrical shock — applied directly to it. This significant discovery removed from physiology the old and harmful idea of animal spirits, which were supposed to be generated in the cent nil nervous system and to cause t he swelling of a muscle during contraction by flowing to it along the connecting nerve. Bui i<> remove a mu jele from t lie body and make it contract by an artificial stimulus does not prove that the muscle substance itself is capable of being acted upon by the stimulus, since in such an experiment tin- endings of the nerve in the muscle an; still intact, and if may be thai the BtimuhlS acts only on them and thus affects the muscle indirectly. In ,-i number of ways, however, physiologists have found thai the muscle substance can be made to contract by a * Sir- \I.ii< v, " l)n mouvemenl dans lea f emotions de In, vie," L868, \>. 284 i Haycraft, " Journal of Physiology," 31, 392, 1904. THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 23 stimulus applied directly to it, and therefore exhibits what is known as independent irritability. The term irritability, according to modern usage, means that a tissue can be made to exhibit its peculiar form of functional activity when stimulated, — e. g., a muscle cell will contract, a gland cell will secrete, etc., — and inde- pendent irritability in the case under consideration means simply that the muscle gives its reaction of contraction when artificial stimuli are applied directly to its substance. This conception of irritability was first introduced by Francis Glisson (1597-1677), a celebrated English physician.* Subsequent writers frequently used the term as synonymous with contractility and as applicable only to the muscle. But it is now used for all living tissues in the sense here indicated. A simple proof of the independent irritability of a striated muscle is obtained by cutting the motor nerve going to it and stimulating the muscle after several days. We know now that in the course of several days the severed nerve fibers degenerate completely down to their terminations in the muscle fibers, and the muscle, thus freed from its nerve fibers by the process of degeneration, can still be made to contract by an artificial stimulus. The classical proof of the independent irri- tability of muscle fibers was given by Claude Bernard, the great French physiologist of the nineteenth century. He made use of the so-called arrow poison of the South American Indians. This substance or mixture of substances is known generally under the name curare; it is prepared from the juices of several plants (strychnos) (Thorpe). The poisonous part of the material is soluble in water, and Bernard showed that when such an extract is injected into the blood or hypodermically it paralyzes the motor nerves at their peripheral end, so that direct stimulation of these nerves is ineffective. Direct stimulation of the muscle substance, on the contrary, causes a contraction, f We are justified, therefore, in saying that skeletal muscle possesses the properties of independ- ent contractility (Haller) and independent irritability (Ber- nard). By the former term we mean that the shortening of the muscle is due to active processes developed in its own tissue, by the latter we mean that the muscular tissue may be made to enter into contraction by artificial stimuli applied directly to its own substance. This latter property cannot be said to hold for all the tissues. Whether a nerve cell or a gland cell may be made to enter into its specific form of activity by the direct application of an artificial stimulus is still an undetermined question. * See Foster's "History of Physiology," p. 287. f "Lemons sur les effets des substances toxiques et medicamenteuses," 1857, pp. 238 et seq. 24 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. Artificial Stimuli. — If we designate the stimulus that the muscle receives normally from its nerve as its normal stimulus, all other forms of energy which may be used to start its contraction may be grouped under the designation artificial stimuli. Experi- ments have shown that a contraction may be aroused by mechani- Fig. 6. — The induction coil as used for physiological purposes (du Bois:Reymond pattern): A, The primary coil; B, the secondary coil; P', binding posts to which are at- tached the wires from the battery, they connect with the ends of cod A; P", binding posts connecting with ends of coil B, through which the induction current is led off; S, the slide, with scale, in which coil B is moved to alter its distance from A. cal stimuli, — for instance, by a sharp blow applied to the muscle; by thermal stimuli, — that is, by a sudden change in temperature; by chemical stimuli, — for example, by the action of concentrated solu- tions of salts, and finally by electrical stimuli. In practice, how- ever, only the last form of stimulus is found to be convenient. The Fig. 7. — Schema of induction apparatus, (Lombard.) b represents 'lie galvanic ' connected by wires to the primary coil, A. On the course of one of these wires is a key (A) to make and break the current, B shows the principle of the secondary coil, and the connection of its two ends with the nerve of a nerve-muscle preparation. When the battery current is closed or made in A, a brief current of high intensitj is induced in H. This is known as the making or cloBmg shock. When the battery current w broken in A, < econd brief induction current i arou ed in B, breaking or opening shock. This is known as the mechanical and thermal stimuli cannol be well applied without at t Ik- same t ime injuring I he muscle subsl ance, and I he same is prob- ably true of chemical stimuli, which possess the disadvantage, more- over, of iioi exciting simultaneously the differenl fibres of which the mu cle i composed. Electrical stimuli, on the contrary, are THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 25 applied easily, are readily controlled as regards their intensity, and affect all the fibers simultaneously, thus giving a co-ordinated contraction of the entire bundle, as is the case with the normal stimulus. For electrical stimulation we may use the galvanic current taken directly from the battery, or the induced or so-called faradic current obtained from an induction coil. Under most conditions the latter is more convenient, since it gives brief shocks, the strength and number of which can be controlled readily. The form in which this instrument is used in experimental work in physiology we owe to du Bois-Reymond (1849-); hence it is frequently known as the du Bois-Reymond induction coil. Experi- mental physiology owes a great deal to this simple and serviceable instrument. A figure and brief description of the apparatus is appended (Figs. 6 and 7). Simple Contraction of Muscle. — Experiments may be made upon the muscles of various animals, but ordinarily in physiolog- ical laboratories one of the muscles (gastrocnemius) of the hind leg of the frog is employed. If such a muscle is isolated and connected with the terminals from an induction coil it may be stimulated by a single shock or by a series of rapidly repeated shocks. The contraction that results from a single stimulus is designated as a simple contraction. In the frog's muscle it is very brief, lasting for 0.1 second or less; but in this, as in other respects, cross-striated muscular tissue varies in different animals,* as is shown by the accompanying table, which gives a general idea of the range of rapidity of contraction: DURATION OF A SIMPLE MUSCULAR CONTRACTION. Insect 0.003 sec. Rabbit (Marey) 0.070 " Frog 0.100 " Terrapin 1.000 " The series may be continued by the figures obtained from the plain muscle, thus: The involuntary muscle (mammal) 10.00 Foot muscle of slugf (Ariolimax) 20.00 The duration of the simple contraction varies considerably in the muscles of different parts of the same animal. Thus, according to Cash, the hyoglossal muscle in the frog requires 0.205 to 0.3 second, while the gastrocnemius takes 0.12 second; in the tortoise the pectoralis major requires 1.8 seconds, the omohyoid only 0.55 second; in the rabbit the soleus (a red * Cash, "Archiv f. Anat. u. Physiol.," 1880, suppl. volume, p. 147. t Carlson, "American Journal of Pysiology," 10, 418, 1904. 2o THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. muscle) requires 1 second, the gastrocnemius (a pale muscle) 0.2."> second. On examining into these differences it may lie shown that the variations bear a relation to the special functions of the muscles. Rapidity of contraction and maintenance of contraction are two properties which are capable of being altered by the processes of adaptation, either together or independently, to suit the needs of the organism. The distribution of the pale and red muscles in such an animal as the rabbit bears out this idea. It will be remembered also that these two varieties show a difference in histological structure (p. 19). The Curve of Contraction. — When a contracting muscle is attached to a lever this lever may be made to write upon a smoked surface and thus record the movement, more or less magnified according to the leverage chosen. If the recording surface is sta- tionary the record obtained is a straight line and indicates only the extent of the shortening. If, however, the recording surface is in movement during the contraction the record will be in the form of a curve, which, making use of the system of right-angled co-ordinates. Fig. 8. — Curve of simple muscular contraction. will indicate ao\ only the full extent of the shortening, but also the amount of shortening or subsequent relaxation at any moment during the entire period. To obtain such records from the rapidly contracting frog's muscle ii is evidenl that the recording surface nni-i move with considerable rapidity and with a uniform velocity. A curve of this kind is represented in Fig. s. C represents the axis Of abscissas and gives the factor of time. A vertical Ordinate erected at any point on C .uives the extent of shortening at that moment. I'.clou the curve of the muscle is the record of the vibration- of a tuning fork giving 100 doulile vibrations econd; thai i-. the di tance from cresl t<> crest represents an interval of ,,',,, of a second. Three principal facts are brought out by an analysis of the curve: b The latent period. By this is THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 27 meant that the muscle does not begin to shorten until a certain time after the stimulus is applied. On the curve the stimulus enters the muscle at S, and the distance between this point and the beginning of the rise of the curve, interpreted in time, is the latent period. II. The phase of shortening, which has a definite course and at its end immediately passes into III., the phase of relaxation. The Latent Period. — In the contraction of the isolated frog's muscles as usually recorded the latent period amounts to 0.01 sec., but it is generally assumed that this period is exaggerated by the method of recording used, since the elasticity of the muscle itself prevents the immediate registration of the movement. By improve- ments in methods of technique the latent period for a fresh muscle may be reduced to as little as 0.005 or even 0.004 sec. Under the conditions in the body, however, the muscle contracts against a load, as when lifting a lever; hence, we may assume that normally there is a lost time of at least 0.01 sec. after the stimulus enters the muscle. In addition to the latent period due to the elasticity of the muscle it is certain that a brief amount of time actually elapses after the stimulus enters the muscle before the act of shortening begins; some time is taken up in the chemical changes and the effect of these changes in putting the mechanism of con- traction into play (see below on the Theory of Muscle Contractions). The latent period varies greatly in muscles of different kinds, and in the same muscle varies with its conditions as regards temperature, fatigue, load to be raised, etc. The Phases of Shortening and of Relaxation. — In the normal frog's muscle the phase of shortening for a simple contraction occu- pies about 0.04 second, while the relaxation may be a trifle longer, 0.05 sec. In muscles whose duration of contraction differs from that of the frog the time values for the shortening and the relaxation exhibit corresponding differences. As we have seen, the appearance of the muscle fiber when viewed by polarized light indicates that during the phase of shortening some of the material in the light bands is imbibed into the substance composing the dim bands. The process is evidently a reversible one; during the phase of relaxation the absorbed or imbibed material passes back to the light band. Many conditions, some of which will be described below, alter the time necessary for these processes, that is, the duration of the simple contraction. It is noteworthy that it is the phase of relaxation which may be most easily prolonged or shortened by varying conditions. Isotonic and Isometric Contractions. — In the method of recording the shortening of the muscle that is described above the muscle is supposed to con- tract against a constant load which it can lift. Such a contraction is spoken of as an isotonic contraction. If the muscle is allowed to contract against 28 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. a tension too great for it to overcome — a stiff spring, for instance — it is prac- tically prevented from shortening, and a contraction of this kind, in -which the length of the muscle remains unchanged, is spoken of as an isometric contraction. A curve of such a contraction may be obtained by magnifying greatly, by means of levers, the slight change in the stiff spring against which the muscle is contracting. Such a curve gives a picture of the liberation of energy within the muscle during contraction. The usual oval form of dynamometer employed to record the grip of the flexors of the fingers gives an isometric record of the energy of contraction of these muscles. Fig. 9. — Effect of varying the strength of stimulus. The figure shows the effect upon the gastrocnemius muscle of a frog of gradually increasing the stimulus (breaking induction siidik) until maximum contractions were obtained. The stimuli were then decreased ID strength and the contractions fell off through a series of gradually decreasing submaximal contractions. '1 ho series up and down is not absolutely regular owing to the difficulty of obtaining a regular increase or decrease in the stimulus. (The prolongations of the curves below the base line are due to the elastic extension of the muscle by the weight dur- ing relaxation.) Effect of Strength of Stimulus upon the Simple Contraction. —The strength of electrical stimuli can be varied conveniently and with greal accuracy. When the stimulus is of such a strength as to produce a just visible contraction it is spoken of as a minimal stimulus and the resulting contraction as a minimal contraction. Stimuli of less strength than the aiinimal are designated as sub- minimal. If one increases gradually the intensity of the electrical current used ae a stimulus without altering its duration, beginning with a stimulus Bufficienl to cause a minimal contraction, the result- ing contraction! increase proportionally up to a certain maximum beyond which furl her increase of si imulus, ol her condit ions remain- ing the same, causes no greater extent of shortening. Contrac- tion! between the minimal and the maximal are designated as submaximal.* (Sei I ig. 9.) * Kick, "i ntersuchungen liber elektrische Nfervenreizung," Braun- • ig, i sm. THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 29 Effect of Temperature upon the Simple Contraction. — Varia- tions in temperature affect both the extent and the duration of the contraction. The relationship is, however, not a simple one in the case of the frog's muscle upon which it has been studied most fre- quently. If we pay attention to the extent of the contraction alone it will be found that at a certain temperature, 0° C, or slightly below, Fig. 10. — Curve showing the effect of temperature. The temperatures at which the contractions were obtained are indicated on the figure. In this experiment a large resis- tance was introduced into the secondary circuit so that changes in the resistance of the muscle itself due to heating could not affect the strength of the stimulus. the muscle loses its irritability entirely. As its temperature is raised a given stimulus, chosen of such a strength as to be maximal for the muscle at room temperatures, causes greater and greater contractions up to a certain maximum, which is reached at about 5° to 9° C. As the temperature rises beyond this point the con- tractions decrease somewhat to a minimum that is reached at about 15° to 18° C. Beyond this the contractions again increase in extent to a second maximum at about 26° to 30° C, this maxi- mum being in some cases greater, and in others less than the first maximum. Beyond the second maximum the contractions again decrease rather rapidly as the temperature rises until at a certain temperature, 37° C, irritability is entirely lost (Fig. 10). If the tem- perature is raised somewhat beyond this latter point heat rigor makes its appearance, and the muscle may be considered as dead. The re- lationship between temperature and extent of contraction, therefore, may be expressed by a curve such as is represented in Fig. 11, in which there are two maxima and two points at which irritability is lost. The second maximum indicates a fact of general physiological in- terest,— namely, that in all of the tissues of the body there is a certain high temperature at which optimum activity is exhibited, and if the temperature is raised beyond this point functional activity becomes 30 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND .NERVE. more and more depressed. The point of optimum effect is not iden- tical for the different tissues of the same animal, much less so for those of different animals, but the fart may be emphasized that in no case do protoplasmic tissues withstand a very high temperature. rr g' 16' Fig. 11. — Curve to show the effect of a temperature from 0° C. to 38° C. upon the height of con tract ion of frog's muscle. The fir.-t maximum at 9° C, the second at 28° C. Beyond 38° C. the muscle lost its irritability and went into rigor mortis. §> Ctx S° 10' 15' 10- 2J- JO- Jtf- 3f jr ir Fig. 12. — Curve to show the effect of a rise of temperature from 5° C. to 39° C. upon the duration of contraction of frog's muscle. The relative dura- tions at the different temperatures are represented by the height of the cor- responding ordinate.?. Functional activity is lost usually at -l.r)°(\ or below. The duration of the contraction shows usually in frogs' muscles a simple relation- ship to the changes of temperature. At low temperatures, 4 or 5° (■., the contractions are enormously prolonged, particularly in the phase of relaxation ; bu1 as the temperature is raised the duration of the contractions diminishes, a1 first rapidly, then more slowly, to a certain point aboul 18° to 20° ( '.. beyond \\ hich LI remains more or constanl in spite of the changes in extent, of shortening. The relationship between duration of contraction and temperature may therefore be expre ed by such a curve as is shown in l"\£. 12, in which the heights of the ordinates represent the relative durations ot the contractions. Muscles from different frogs show considerable minor variations in their reactions to changes in temperature, and we may Buppose that these variations depend upon differences in THE PHENOMENON OP CONTRACTION. 31 nutritive condition. In this, as in many other respects, the reactions obtained from so-called winter frogs after they have prepared for hibernation are more regular and typical than those obtained in the spring or summer. Effect of Veratrin. — The alkaloid veratrin exhibits a peculiar and interesting effect upon the contraction of muscle. A muscle taken from an animal that has been veratrinized and stimulated in the usual way by a single stimulus gives a contraction such as is exhibited in the accompanying curve (Fig. 13). Two peculiarities are shown by the curve: (1) The phase of shortening is not altered, but the phase of relaxation is greatly prolonged. (2) The curve Pig. 13. — Curve showing the effect of veratrin. shows two summits, — that is, after the first shortening there is a brief relaxation followed by a second, slower contraction. The cause of this second shortening is not known. Biedemann has suggested that it is due to the presence in the muscle of the two kinds of fibers — red and pale — which were spoken of on p. 26, and that the veratrin dissociates their action, but this expla- nation, according to Carvallo and Weiss,* is disproved by the fact that muscles composed entirely of white or red fibers show a similar result from the action of veratrin. Although the explana- tion is not forthcoming, the fact that a single stimulus gives under these conditions two processes of contractions is interesting as an exception to the general rule. It may be added that a curarized frog's muscle, when heated to the point of optimum activity (28° C), sometimes shows also a double contraction for a single stimulus. The very prolonged relaxation is, however, the most peculiar effect of the veratrin. A somewhat similar effect is produced by the action of glycerin. We have in such substances reagents that affect one phase of the contraction process without materially influencing the other. As regards the veratrin effect, it becomes less and less marked if the muscle is made to give repeated contractions, but reappears after a suitable period of rest. The peculiar action of the veratrin is, threrefore, antagonized seemingty by the chemical products formed during contraction. * "Journal de la physiol. et de la path, generate," 1899. 32 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. Contracture. — The prolonged relaxation that is so character- istic of the veratrinized muscles may be observed in frog's muscle under other circumstances, and is described usually as a con- >.J3 01 £ tea "E-S C w dit ion of contracture. By contracture we mean a slate of maintained contraction or, Looking al it from the other point of view, a state of retarded relaxation. THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 33 This condition is often exhibited in a most interesting way when a muscle is repeatedly stimulated. In some cases it develops at the beginning of a series of contractions, as is represented in Fig. 14, which pictures the phenome- non as it was first described.* In other cases it appears later on in the curve, 3 ■B'SlSS! fa WMiXuW'Wm ! WmmMm^ THB pw$IK ",'■. '''A, •*•■■' ■■■;' ■ :Smmm» JSrTOSSBMlllMBUi jhk^^^fll Fig. 15. — Effect of repeated stimulation; complete curve, showing late contracture. The muscle was stimulated by induction shocks at the rate of 50 per minute. The separate contractions are so close together that they can not be distinguished. preceding or following the development of the state of fatigue. Whenever it occurs the effect is to hold the muscle in a state of maintained contraction, on which is superposed the series of quick contractions and relaxations due to the separate stimuli. When the condition develops early in the functional activity of the muscle (Fig. 14) further activity usually causes it to disappear, Fig. 16. — Effect of repeated stimulation, curve showing no contracture or very little. The muscle was stimulated by induction shocks at the rate of 50 per minute. A very slight contracture is shown in the beginning, but subsequently the contractions show only a diminished extent, the rate of relaxation remaining apparently unchanged. and the condition of the muscle as a mechanism for prompt shortening and relaxation is improved. We have in this fact apparently an indication of one way in which the "warming up" exercise before athletic contests may be of value. When the contraction appears late in the series of contractions * Tiegel, "Pfliiger's Archivfur die gesammte Physiologie, " etc., 13, 71, 1876. 3 34 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. it is usually permanent, that is to say, it wears off only as the muscle relaxes slowly from fatigue. Toward the end of such a series the muscle is often practically in a state of continuous contraction, a condition which would nullify its ordinary use in locomotion. It seems possible that certain conditions of tonic spasm or cramps which occur during life may involve this process, for example, the temporary cram]) that sometimes attacks a player in athletic games, or the curious spasmodic condition known as intermittent claudication, in which, apparently as a result of insufficient circulation, the muscles on exercise are thrown into a state of tonic contraction. From the physiological standpoint the phenomenon of contracture when compared with that of the simple contraction indicates the possibility that two different contraction processes may take place in muscle, one involving the state of tone and. therefore, the length and hardness of the muscle, the other con- trolling the movements proper. This suggestion has been made by a number of authors* on various grounds, and is of interest as indicating a point of view different from that usually held in regard to muscular contraction. The Effect of Rapidly Repeated Contractions. — When a muscle is stimulated repeatedly by stimuli of equal strength that fall into the muscle at equal intervals the contractions show certain features that, in a general way, are constant, although the precise degree in' which they are exhibited varies curiously in different animals. Such curves are exhibited in Figs. 14, 15, and 16, and the features worthy of note may be specified briefly as .follows: 1. The Introductory Contractions. — The first three or four con- tractions decrease slightly in extent, showing that the muscle at first loses a little in irritability on account of previous contractions. This phenomenon is frequently absent. 2. The Staircase or " Treppe." — After the first slight fall in height has passed off the contractions increase in extent with great regularity and often for a surprisingly large number of contractions. This gradual increase in extent of shortening, with a constant stimulus, was first noticed by Bowditch upon the heart muscle, and was by him named the phenomenon of "treppe," the German word for staircase. It indicates that the effect of activity is in the beginning beneficial to the muscle in that its irritability steadily increases, and the fact that the same result has been ob- tained from heart muscle, plain muscle, and nerve fibers indicates that it may be a general physiological law that functional activity leads at first to a heightened irritability. According to Lee,f the " treppe " in muscle is due to an initial augmenting effect of the chemical products formed during contraction. .'). ( 'on Intel lire. This phenomenon of maintained contraction Ij.i- been described above. In frog's muscles Stimulated repeat- edly it makes its appearance, as a rule, sooner or Later in llie Series of contractions; but there is a curious amount of variation in the muscles of different individuals in this respect. • See especially [Texkull, "Zentralblatl f. Physiologie," L908, 22, 33; also Guenther, "American Journal of Physiology," L905, L4, 7.'!. t Bee "American Journal of Physiology," 1907, L8, 267. THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 35 4. Fatigue. — After the period of the " treppe " has passed, the contractions diminish steadily in height, until at last the muscle fails entirely to respond to the stimulus. This progressive loss of irritability in the muscle caused by repeated activity is designated as fatigue. It will be considered more in detail under the head of Compound Muscular Contractions and in Chapter II. The curve obtained in an experiment of this kind illustrates in a striking way one of the general characteristics of living matter, namely, that every effective stimulus applied to it leaves a record, so to speak. The muscle in this case is in a changed condition after each stimulus, as is indicated by the difference in its re- sponse to the succeeding stimulus. While it cannot be said that a similar effect has been shown in all tissues, still the evidence in general points that way, and some of the complicated phenomena exhibited by living matter, such as memory, habits, immunity, etc., are referable in the long run to this underlying peculiarity. Lee has discovered the interesting fact that while in frog's muscle, as a rule, fatigue is accompanied by a prolongation of the curve, especially of the phase of relaxation, this does not hold for mammalian muscle. In the latter muscle the successive contractions become smaller as fatigue sets in, but their duration is not increased. The Contraction Wave. — Under ordinary conditions the fibers of a muscle when stimulated contract simultaneously or nearly so, and the whole extent of the muscle is practically in the same phase of contraction at a given instant. It is comparatively easy to show, however, that the process of contraction spreads over the fibers, from the point stimulated, in the form of a wave which moves with a definite velocity. In a long muscle with parallel fibers one may prove, by proper recording apparatus, that if the muscle is stimulated at one end a point near this end enters into contraction before a point farther off. Knowing the difference in time between the appearance of the contraction at the two points and the dis- tance apart of the latter, we have the data for determining the velocity of its propagation. In frog's muscles this velocity is found to be equal to 3 to 4 meters per second, while in human muscle, at the body temperature, it is estimated at 10 to 13 meters per second. Knowing the time it takes this wave to pass a given point (d) and its velocity (v), its entire length is given by the formula I = vd. In the frog's muscle, therefore, with a velocity of 3000 mm. per second, and a duration of, say, 0.1 second, the product (3000X0.1=300 mms.) gives the length of the wave or the length of muscle which is in some phase of contraction at any given instant. Under normal conditions the muscle fibers are stimulated through their motor plates, which are situated toward the middle of the fiber, or perhaps one muscle fiber may have 36 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. two or more motor plates, giving two or more points of stimula- tion. It follows, therefore, from this anatomical arrangement and the great velocity of the wave that all parts of the fibers are in contraction at the same instant and, indeed, in nearly the same phase of contraction. Under abnormal conditions muscles may exhibit fibrillar contractions; that is, separate fibrils or bundles of fibrils contract and relax at different times, giving a flickering, trembling movement to the muscle. Idiomuscular Contractions. — In a fatigued or moribund muscle mechan- ical stimulation may give a localized contraction which does not spread or spreads very slowly, showing that the abnormal changes in the muscle prevent the excitation from traveling at its normal velocity. A localized contraction of this kind was designated by Schiff as an idiomuscular contraction. It may be produced in the muscle of a dying or recently dead animal by localized mechanical stimulation, as by drawing a blunt instrument e. single stimulu . the latent period being indicated at the beginning of the contraction. Curve - show- th ti in due to two succeeding stimuli. stimuli at varying intervals. If the second stimulus falls into the muscle at the apex of the contraction caused by the first stimulus, then, even if t he firs! conl racl ion is maximal, I tie muscle will shorten si ill farther; the firsl and second contractions are summated, giv- ing .-i total shortening greater than can be obtained by a single stim- ulus (see Fig. 20). The extenl of the summation in such cases varie ■'. 1 ' h b n umbel- of conditions, such as the intervals bel ween the THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 43 stimuli, the relative strengths of the stimuli, the load carried by the muscle, etc. Taking the simplest conditions of a moderately loaded muscle and two maximal stimuli, it is found that the greatest sum- mation occurs when the stimuli are so spaced that the second contrac- tion begins at the apex of the first. If the stimuli are closer together, so that, for instance, the second contraction follows shortly after the first has begun, the total shortening is less, and the same is true to an increasing extent as the second contraction falls later and later in the period of relaxation after the first contraction.* If instead of two we use three successive stimuli, falling into the muscle at proper intervals, a still further summation occurs. In this way the total extent of shortening in a muscle completely tetanized may be several times as great as that of a single maximal contraction. The Discontinuous Character of the Tetanic Contraction — The Muscle-tone. — In complete tetanus the muscle seems to be in a condition of continuous uniform contraction; the re- corded curve shows no sign of relaxation between stimuli and no external indication, in fact, that the separate stimuli do more than maintain a state of uniform contraction. It can be shown, how- ever, that in reality each stimulus has its own effect, and that the chemical changes underlying the phenomenon of contraction form an interrupted series corresponding, within limits, to the series of stimuli sent in. The clearest proof for this belief is found in the electrical changes that result from each stimulus, and the facts relating to this side of the question will be stated subsequently in the chapter on The Electrical Phenomena of Muscle and Nerve. Another proof is found in the phenome- non of the muscle-tone. When a muscle is stimulated directly or through its motor nerve a musical note may be heard by applying the ear or a stethoscope to the muscle. The note that is heard corresponds in pitch, up to a certain point, with the num- ber of stimuli sent in,— that is, the muscle vibrates, as it were, in unison with the number of stimuli, and, although the vibrations are not sufficient to affect the recording lever, they can be heard as a musical note. This fact, therefore, may be taken as a proof that during complete tetanus there is a discontinuous series of changes in the rhuscle the rate of which corresponds with that of the stimulation. The series of electrical changes corresponding with the series of stimuli sent in may be made audible by applying a telephone to the muscle. Making use of this method, Wedenskif has shown that the ability of the muscle to respond isorhythmically to the rate of stimulation is limited. In frog's muscle the pitch of the * Von Kries, "Archiv fur Physiologie," 1888, p. 537. t Wedenski, " Du rhythme musculaire dans la contraction normale," "Archives de physiologie," 1891, p. 58. 44 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. musical tone may correspond with the rate of stimulation up to about 200 stimuli per second. In the muscle of the warm-blooded animal the correspondence may extend to about 1000 stimuli per second. If the rate of stimulation is increased beyond these limits the musical note heard does not correspond, but falls to a lower pitch, indicating that some of the stimuli under these conditions become ineffective. It should be added that the high figures given above for the correspondence between the stimuli and the muscle-tone hold good only for entirely fresh preparations. The lability of the muscle quickly becomes less as it is fatigued; so that in the frog, for instance, the correspondence in long-continued contractions is accurate only when the rate of stimulation does not exceed 30 per second. The Number of Stimuli Necessary for Complete Tetanus. — The number of stimuli necessary to produce complete tetanus varies, as we should expect, with the kind of muscle used and in accordance with the rapidity of the process of relaxation shown by these muscles in simple contractions. The series that may be arranged to demonstrate this variation is quite large, extending from a supposed rate of 300 per second for insect muscle to a low limit of one stimulus in 5 to 7 seconds for plain muscle. The frog's muscle goes into complete tetanus with a rate of stimulation of from 20 to 30 per second. Inasmuch as the rapidity of relaxation of the muscle is much retarded by certain influences, such as a low temperature or fatigue, it follows that these same influences affect in a corresponding way the rate of stimulation necessary to give complete tetanus. A frog's muscle stimulated at the rate of 10 stimuli per second may record an incomplete tetanus, but if the stimulus is maintained for some time the tetanus finally becomes complete in consequence of the slowing of the phase of relaxation. Voluntary Contractions.- After ascertaining that muscles may give either simple or tetanic contractions one asks naturally whether in our voluntary movements we can also obtain both sorts of contractions. In the first place, it is obvious that most of our voluntary movements are too long continued to be simple contractions. The time element alone would place them in the group of tetanic emit met ions, and this is the usual conclusion regarding them. In voluntary movements a neuromuscular mechanism comes into play. This mechanism consists, on the motor side, of at least two nerve units or neurons and the muscle, as indie., led in the accompanying diagram (Fig. 21). If in ordi- nary voluntary movements the muscular contractions are tetanic, we imi-i suppose that the motor nerve cells discharge a series of nerve impulses through the motor nerve into the muscle. The contraction of voluntary muscle has been investigated, therefore, THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 45 Fig. 21. — Schema to show the innerva- tion of the skeletal (voluntary) muscles: 1, the intercentral (pyramidal) neuron; 2, the spinal neuron; 3, the muscle. in various ways to ascertain whether there is any objective indi- cation of the number of separate contractions that are fused together to make this normal tetanus. In the first place, the normal movements of the muscles have been recorded graphically by levers or tambours. The records thus obtained show that our usual contractions are not entirely complete tetani — that is, there is an indication in some part of the curve of the single con- tractions that are being fused. According to most observers,* these records show that our normal contractions are com- pounded of single contrac- tions following at the rate of 10 per second, or, in other words, the motor neurons discharge about 10 impulses per second into the muscle. The so-called natural muscle- tone has been used for the same purpose. When one places a stethoscope or lays his ear upon a contracting muscle a low tone is heard, the pitch of which corresponds with 40 vibrations per second. It was formerly assumed that this note does not represent the actual rate of stimulation of the muscle, since the number is higher than that obtained by some other methods. A rate of 35 to 40 vibrations per second corresponds to the resonance tone of the external ear and it is possible that the real muscle tone may have a lower pitch, and that the ear picks out by its own resonance one of the overtones. Helmholtz made use of a simple and direct method to determine this point. He utilized the principle of sympathetic vibrations, according to which a vibrating body will be set into movement most easily by vibrations that correspond in number to its own period. Helmholtz attached to the muscle watch springs that had different periods of vibration, and found that when the muscle was contracted the spring that vibrated 20 times per second Was set into most active movement. He concluded, therefore, that the muscle receives 20 stimuli per second in ordinary con- tractions and that the tone that is heard, 40 vibrations per second, represents the first overtone. The whole subject has been reinvestigated more recently by employing the "string galvanometer" (see p. 100) to record the number of electrical *Horsley and Schafer, "Journal of Physiology," 7, 96, 1886. 46 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. variations occurring during a voluntary contraction. Since each separate stimulus to a muscle causes a distinct electrical variation, it is evident that if we can record the number of such variations per second we shall have almost conclusive evidence as regards the number of simple contractions which enter into the production of voluntary tetanus. The string galvanometer lends itself to this purpose better than any form of electrometer yet devised, and Piper,* by the use of this instrument, finds that in voluntary contractions of the flexor muscles of the arms or fingers the number of electrical variations follow at the rate of 47 to 50 per second. Increase in strength of contraction in , •U-. Wmi. m Fig. 22. — I'he upper curve shows the vibrations of the "'string" of the string gal- vanometer during voluntary contraction of the flexor of the fingers. Each vibration is due to an electrical oscillation in the muscle (action current). These oscillations occur at the rate of 50 per second, as may be seen by reference to the lower curve, the breaks in which indicate fifths of a second. This fact would indicate, therefore, that in the voluntary con- traction we have a tetanus composed of single contractions following at the rate of 50 per second. — (From Piper.) these muscles causes no change in rate, although a corresponding variation in the intensity of the electrical changes is observed. When different muscles are studied by this method, quite a marked difference in rate is obtained. Piper reports such observations as the following: M. deltoideus, 58 to 62; M. gas- trocnemius and M. tibialis anterior, 42 to 44; M. quadriceps femoris, 38 to 41; M. masseter, 88 to 100, and M. temporalis, SO to 86. Assuming that these figures represent the rate of dis- charge of nerve impulses per second by the nerve cells from which arise the motor fibers to the muscles named, it is evident that the various spinal and cranial motor centers may possess quite widely different rhythms, although for each particular center the rate is more or less fixed. Among the motor centers thus far studied it will be noted that the cells of the N. trigeminus possess the highest rate of discharge. There lias been much discussion as to whether or not we can obtain simple as well as * Piper, PflOger'a "Archiv f. d. ges Physiologic," L907, 119,301. Also "Zeitachrifl f. Biologic," L908, 60, 393, and 504. THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 47 compound contractions by voluntary stimulation of our muscles. It has been pointed out that in very rapid contractions, such as occur in the trilling movements of the fingers in playing the piano, the duration of the separate contractions is so brief as to suggest that they may be of the order of simple contractions. Direct investigation of such movements by the older method of recording with levers (von Kries) or by the newer method of photographing the electrical oscillations shows, on the contrary, that even the shortest possible voluntary contractions are brief tetani made up of a short lasting series of contractions fused together. In all probability, therefore, our motor centers, when- ever they are stimulated by a so-called act of the will, discharge rhythmically a series of nerve impulses. As we shall see later, it is possible that certain of these centers, when stimulated reflexly, may discharge a single nerve impulse and thus arouse a simple muscular contraction (see Knee-kick). Fig. 23. — Mosso's ergograph: c is the carriage moving to and fro on runners by means of the cord d, which passes from the carriage to a holder attached to the last two phalanges of the middle finger (the adjoining fingers are held in place by clamps) ; p, the writing point of the carriage, c, which makes the record of its movements on the kymographion ; w, the weight to be lifted. The Ergograph. — Voluntary contractions in man may be re- corded in a great many ways, but Mosso has devised a special in- strument for this purpose, known as the ergograph. It has been much used in quantitative investigations upon muscular work and the conditions influencing it. The apparatus is shown and described in Fig. 23. The person experimented upon makes a series of short contractions of the flexor muscle of the middle finger, thereby lifting a known weight to a definite height which is recorded upon a drum. In a set of experiments the 48 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. rate of the series of contractions — that is, the interval of rest between the contractions — is kept constant, as also is the load lifted. Under these conditions the contractions become less and less ex- tensive as fatigue comes on. and finally, with the strongest voluntary effort, the contraction of the muscles is insufficient to lift the weight. In this way a record is obtained such as is shown in Fig. 24. In such a record we can easily calculate the total work done by obtaining the product of the load into the lift for each contrac- tion and adding these products together. By this means the capacity for work of the muscle used can be studied objectively under varying conditions, and many suggestive results have been 1 ig, _' t . — Norm al fatigue curve of the flexors of the middle finger of right hand. '.i kilograms, contractions at intervals of two seconds. — (Matjgiora.) Weight obtained, some of which will be referred to specifically.* It should be borne in mind, however, thai the ergograph in this form does not enable ufi to compute the total work that the muscle is capable of performing. It is obvious thai when the poinl of complete fatigue is reached, as Illustrated in the record, Fig. 23, the muscle is still capable of doing work, thai is external work, if we replace the heavy load by a lighter one. For this reason some investigators have substituted a spring in place of the load,f giving thus a spring ergograph instead of a weight ergograph. Although with the *Mosso, " \rrliivc-. italiennee de biologic," L3, 187, L890; :ils<> "Archivf. JM.y aologie," 1899, p. L91, 342. Lombard, " Journal of Physiology," 13, I, 1892. f Franz, " American Journal of Physiology," 4, .'i is, moo; ulso Hough, (bid., ■',. 240, 1901. THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 49 spring ergograph every muscular contraction is recorded and the entire work done may be calculated, it also possesses certain theo- retical and practical disadvantages, for a discussion of which refer- ence must be made to the authors quoted. The weight ergograph has, so far at least, given us the most sug- gestive results. Among these the following may be mentioned: (1) If a sufficient interval is allowed between contractions no fatigue is apparent. With a load of 6 kilograms, for instance, the flexor muscle (M. flexor digitorum sublimis) showed no fatigue when a rest of 10 seconds was given between contractions. (2) After complete fatigue with a given load a very long interval (two hours) is necessary for the muscle to make a complete recovery and give a second record as extensive as the first. (3) After complete fatigue efforts to still further contract the muscle greatly prolong this period of complete recovery, — a fact that demonstrates the injurious effect of straining a fatigued muscle. (4) The power of a muscle to do work is diminished by conditions that depress the general nutritive state of the body or the local nutrition of the muscle used; for instance, by loss of sleep, hunger, mental activity, anemia of the muscle, etc. (5) On the contrary, improved circulation in the muscle — produced by massage, for example — increases the power to do work. Food also has the same effect, and some particularly interesting experiments show that sugar, as a soluble and easily absorbed foodstuff, quickly increases the amount of muscular work that can be performed. .(6) Marked activity in one set of muscles — the use of the leg muscles in long walks, for example — will diminish the amount of work obtainable from other muscles, such as those of the arm. It is very evident that the instrument may be used to advantage in the investigation of many problems connected with gymnastics, dietetics, stimulants,*- medicines, etc. A point of general physiological interest that has been brought out in con- nection with the use of the ergograph calls for a few words of special mention. Mosso found that if a muscle — e. g., the flexor digitorum sublimis — is stimu- lated directly by the electrical current and its contractions are recorded by the ergograph, it will give a curve similar to that figured above for the volun- tary contractions, except that the contractions are not so extensive. Under these conditions the muscle, when completely fatigued to electrical stimula- tion, will respond to voluntary stimulation from the nerve centers. It seems likely, as suggested by Hough, that this result is due mainly to the fact that the electrical current cannot be applied to a muscle in its normal position so as to excite uniformly all the constituent muscle fibers, although it is also possible that what we call the normal or voluntary stimulus is more effective or, to use a physiological term, more adequate to the muscle fibers than the electrical shock. On the other hand, after fatigue from a series of voluntary contractions it has been observed that the muscle will still * Schumberg, "Archiv f. Physiol.," 1899, suppl. volume, p. 289. 4 50 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. give contractions If stimulated directly by electricity. This fact has been interpreted to mean that, in the neuromuscular complex involved in a mus- cular contraction — namely, motor nerve cell, motor nerve fiber, and muscle fiber — the first named fatigues most easily, and that the ordinary fatigue curve obtained from the ergograph does not represent pure muscle fatigue, but fatigue of the neuromuscular apparatus as a whole, the point of complete fatigue being reached in the neural component of the mechanism before the muscle itself loses its power of contraction. This interpretation, however, is not entirely certain. Wedenski has called attention to the fact that in the neuromuscular apparatus the motor end-plate is a sensitive link in the chain, and that, when the nerve is stimulated strongly with artificial stimuli at least, this structure falls into a condition in which it fails to conduct the nerve impulse to the muscle. It may be, therefore, that in sustained volun- tary contractions the end-plate fails first, and thus is directly responsible for the failure of the apparatus to perform further work. That the fatigue in ordinary voluntary contractions affects the muscles before the motor nerve centers is indicated by the experiments of Storey.* Making use of a weight ergograph and experimenting tipon the abductor indicis he found that after fatiguing this muscle to voluntary contractions with a certain weight, re- moval of the weight enabled the individual to make contractions as high and as rapid as before the fatigue. On the other hand, if after removing the weight the muscle was stimulated electrically the contractions were lower and slower than before the fatigue. So far as our knowledge goes, therefore, fatigue as it appears in sustained voluntary contractions is due probably primarily to a loss of irritability in the muscle and in the motor end-plates. The motor nerve fibers do not fatigue, and as regards the motor nerve centers it is not possible as yet to say what may be their relative susceptibility to fatigue. Sense of Fatigue. — It should be noted in passing that in con- tinued voluntary contractions we are conscious of a sense of fatigue, which eventually leads us, if possible, to discontinue our efforts. This sensation must arise from a stimulation of sensory nerve fibers within the muscle or its tendons, and it may be regarded as an important regulation whereby we are prevented from pushing our muscular exertions to the point of " straining." Muscle Tonus, — In addition to the conditions of contraction and of relaxation the living muscle exhibits the phenomenon of "tone." By muscle tone we mean a state of continuous shortening or contraction which under normal conditions is slight in extent and varies from time to time. This condition is dependent upon the connection of the muscle with the nerve centers, and we may assume that under normal circumstances the motor centers are continually discharging subminimal nerve impulses into the muscles which cause chemical changes similar in kind to those set up by an ordinary voluntary effort, but less in degree; the result being thai the muscles enter into a, slate of contraction which, while slight in exlent , is more or less continuous. According lo this view, the whole neuromuscular apparatus is in a condition of tonic ac- tivity, and this slide may be referred in Hie long run to the con- t inual inflow of sensory impulses into the centra] nervous system. That is, I lie tonus <>f the skeletal muscles is not. only dependent * Story, "American Journal of Physiology," L903, 8, '■'<-^>. THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 51 on the nerve centers (neurogenic), but is in reality an example of reflex stimulation of these centers. The tone of any particular muscle or group of muscles may be destroyed, therefore, by cut- ting its motor nerve, or less completely by severing the sensory paths from the same region. If, for instance, one severs in a dog the posterior roots of the spinal nerves innervating the leg, there will be a distinct loss of muscular tone, although the motor nerves remain intact. The underlying cause of tone is poorly understood. It may be, as implied above, simply a condition of subdued tetanus due to a constantly acting series of sub-minimal stimuli, or it may be an order of contraction quite different from the usual visible movements; that is to say, the shortening in the case of tonus may be due to a substance or mechanism in the muscle-fibers different from that which subserves the ordinary quick movements which we designate as contractions. However this may be, the fact of muscle tone is important in a number of ways. It is of value, without doubt, for the normal nutrition of the muscle, and, as is explained in the chapter on Animal Heat, it plays a very important part in controlling the production of heat in the body. The extent of muscle tone varies with many conditions, the most important of which, perhaps, are external temperature and mental activity. With regard to the first, it is known that, as the external temperature falls and the skin becomes chilled, the sensory stimulation thus produced acts upon the nerve centers and leads to an increased discharge along the motor paths to the muscle. The tone of the muscles increases and may pass into the visible movements of shivering. By this means the production of heat within the body is increased automatically. Similarly, an increase in mental activity, so-called mental concentration, whether of an emotional or an intellectual kind, leads, by its effect on the spinal motor centers, to a state of greater muscle tonus, the increased muscular tension being, indeed, visible to our eyes. The Condition of Rigor. — When the muscle substance dies it becomes rigid, or goes into a condition of rigor: it passes from a viscous to a solid state. The rigor that appears in the muscles after somatic death is designated usually as rigor mortis, since its oc- currence explains the death stiffening in the cadaver. It is charac- terized by several features : the muscles become rigid, they shorten, they develop an acid reaction, and they lose their irritability to stimuli. Whether all of these features are necessary parts of the condition of rigor mortis it is difficult to say; the matter will be discussed briefly below. Some of the facts which have been ob- served regarding rigor mortis are as follows: After the death of an individual the muscles enter into rigor mortis at different times. 52 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. Usually there is a certain sequence, the order given being the jaws neck, trunk, upper limbs, lower limbs, the rigor taking, therefore, a descending course. The actual time of the appearance of the rigidity varies greatly, however; it may come on within a few minutes or a number of hours may elapse before it can be detected, the chief de- termining factor in this respect being the condition of the muscle itself. Death after great muscular exertion, as in the case of hunted animals or soldiers killed in battle, is usually followed quickly by muscle rigor; indeed, in extreme cases it may develop almost imme- diately. Death after wasting diseases is also followed by an early Pig. 25. — Curve of normal rigor mortis, gastrocnemius muscle of frog. The curve was obtained upon a kymographion making one revolution in eight days. The murks on the line below the curve indicate interval, of six hours. It will be seen that the shortening required eighteen hours, the relaxation about seventy-two hours. rigor, which in this case is of a more feeble character and shorter duration. The development of rigor is very much hastened by many drugs that bring about the rapid death of the muscle substance, such as veratrin, hydrocyanic acid, caffein, and chloroform. A frog's mus- cle exposed to chloroform vapor goes into rigor at once and shortens to a remarkable extent. Rigor is said also to occur more rapidly in a muscle si ill connected with the central nervous system than in one whose molor nerve has been severed. After a certain interval, which also varies greatly,— Iron i one to six days in human beings,— the rigidity passes off, the muscles again become soft ;nid flexible; this phenomenon is known as the release from rigor. In the cold-blooded animals the development of rigor is very much slower than in warm-blooded animals, Upon an isolated frog's muscle the most striking fact regarding rigor mortis is the shortening that the muscle undergoes. This shortening or contraction comes on lowly, as is shown in the accompanying figure, bul in extent ii exceed the simple contraction obtainable from the living muscle by mean- of a maxima] stimulus. This pari of the phenomenon i . however, much less marked apparently in mammalian muscle, THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 53 and Folin * states that, if rigor be caused in frog's muscle by lowering its temperature to — 15° C, the muscle becomes rigid merely without undergoing any shortening or change in translu- cency. The usual explanation that is given of rigor is that it is due to a coagulation of the fluid substance, the muscle plasma, of which the fibers are constituted. During life the proteins exist in a liquid or viscous condition; after death they coagulate into a solid form. This view is referred to again in the chapter dealing with the chemistry of muscle and nerve; it has received much support from the investigations of Kiihne,f who proved that the muscle plasma is really coagulable. After first freezing and mincing the muscles he succeeded in squeezing out the plasma from the living fibers and showed that it subsequently clotted. While the coagulation theory of rigor explains the greater rigidity of the muscle, it does not furnish in itself a satisfactory explanation of the shortening, and the fact, as stated above, that the rigidity may occur without the shortening indicates that this latter process may possibly be due to changes that precede the appearance of rigidity. In addition to the rigor mortis that occurs after death at ordinary temperatures, a condition of rigor may be induced rapidly by raising the temperature of the muscle to a certain point. Rigor induced in this way is designated as heat rigor or rigor caloris. Much uncertainty has prevailed as to whether heat rigor is different essentially from death rigor. According to some physiologists, the processes may be regarded as the same, the heat rigor being simply a death rigor that is rapidly developed by the high temperature, this latter condition accelerating the chemical changes leading to rigor, as is the case, for instance, in the action of chloroform. This view is supported by a study of the chemical changes that take place under the two conditions, as will be described later, and by the fact that some of the conditions that influence one phenomenon have a parallel effect upon the other. For instance, death rigor is accel- erated by previous use of the muscle, and the same is true for heat rigor. While a resting frog's muscle begins to go into heat rigor, as judged by the shortening, at 37° to 40° C; a muscle that has been greatly fatigued shows the same phenomenon at 25° to 27° C.J According to other observers, heat rigor is due to an ordinary heat coagulation of the proteins present in the muscle fiber, and it has been claimed that a separate contraction may be obtained on heating for each of the proteins said to exist in the muscle fiber. § More recent observations || seem to show * "American Journal of Physiology," 9, 374, 1903. f Kiihne, " Archiv f. Physiologie," 1859, p. 788. X Latimer, "American Journal of Physiology," 2, 29, 1899. § Brodie and Richardson, "Philosophical Trans., Roy. Soc," London, 1899, 191, p. 127; also Inagaki, "Zeitschrift f. Biol.," 1906, 48, 313. || Vrooman, "Bio-chemical Journal," 1907, 2, 363. 54 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. that when a frog's muscle is gradually heated, only two really distinct contractions are obtained, one at 39° C. (38° to 40°) or slightly lower, and one at 50° C. (49° to 51°). Mammalian muscle gives also two contractions when heated, one at 47° C. (46° to 50°) and one at 62° C. (61° to 64°). In each of these cases the second contraction is due to the action of heat on the connective-tissue elements of the muscle. The first contraction is, therefore, the one that is characteristic of the muscular substance proper and the one that marks the occurrence of heat rigor. At the tempertures stated, 39° C. for frog's muscle and 47° C. for mammalian muscle, the viscous material within the sarco- lemma coagulates. While the end result in this case, that is, the coagulation, is the same as occurs in death rigor due to the interruption of the normal blood-supply, nevertheless the causes bringing about the coagulation are different and possibly the properties of the coagulum formed may be different. Ordinary death rigor passes off after a certain interval even if putrefactive processes are excluded; the rigor from heat or from chloroform, on the contrary, shows no release. With regard to the specific cause of the coagulation of death rigor nothing final can be. said. The interesting researches of Fletcher and Hopkins* indicate that during the survival period between the loss of the normal circulation and the appearance of rigor chemical changes are going on in the living substance which result in the formation and accumulation of lactic acid. When the process of production of the lactic acid ceases, the muscle has lost its irritability and then soon enters into the state of rigor. If during this survival period the muscle is kept well supplied with oxygen no lactic acid accumulates in the muscle, and when the muscle finally loses its irritability no rigor occurs. These facts would seem to implicate the lactic acid in some way in the process of clotting and of rigor. Rigor of muscles may be caused, of course, by other specific conditions which kill the muscle and bring on coagulation of the muscle-substance; by the action of distilled water, for example, the so-called water rigor, or by the action of .in excess of calcium salts, calcium rigor. PLAIN OR SMOOTH MUSCULAR TISSUE. Occurrence and Innervation.- Plain or long striated mus- cular ii 3Ue OCCUrS in the walls of all the SO-Called hollow viscera of the body, such as the arteries and veins, the alimentary canal, the genital and urinary organs, the bronchi, etc., and in other special Localities, such as the intrinsic muscles of the eyeball, the * Fletcher and Bopkina, "journal of Phyaiology," 1907, 35, 247. THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 55 muscles attached to the hair follicles, etc. In structure it differs fundamentally from cross-striated muscle, in that it occurs in the form of relatively minute cells, each with a single nucleus, which are united to form, in most cases, muscular membranes constituting a part of the walls of the hollow viscera. These muscle cells, in most cases at least, are supplied with nerve fibers which originate directly from the so-called sympathetic nerve cells, and only in- directly, therefore, from the central nervous system. Speaking generally, the contractions of this tissue are removed from the direct control of the will, being regulated by reflex and usually unconscious stimulations from the central nervous system. All the important movements of the internal organs, or, as they are sometimes called, the organs of vegetative life, are effected through the activity of this contractile tissue. From this stand- point their function may be regarded as more important than that of the mass of the voluntary musculature, since so far as the mere maintenance of the life of the organism is concerned, the proper action and co-ordination of the movements of the visceral organs is at all times essential. Distinctive Properties. — The phenomena of contraction shown by plain muscles are, in general, closely similar to those already studied for striated muscle, the one great difference being the' much greater sluggishness of the changes. Plain muscles differ among themselves, of course, as do the striated muscles, but, speak- ing generally, the simple contractions of plain muscle have a very long latent period that may be a hundred or five hundred times as long as that of cross-striated muscle, and the phases of shortening and of relaxation are also similarly prolonged; so that the whole movement of contraction is relatively slow and gentle (see Fig. 26). Plain muscle responds to artificial stimuli, but the electrical current is obviously a less adequate — that is, a less normal — stimulus for this tissue than for the striped muscle. The amount of current necessary to make it contract is far greater. The amount of con- traction varies with the strength of stimulus, — that is, the tissue gives submaximal and maximal contractions. Two successive stimuli properly spaced will cause a larger or summated contraction, and a series of stimuli will give a fused or tetanic contraction. The rate of stimulation necessary to produce tetanus is, of course, much slower than for cross-striped muscle. The stomach muscle of the frog, for instance, requires only one stimulus at each five sec- onds to cause tetanus.* A distinguishing and important charac- teristic of the plain muscle is its power to remain in tone, — that * Schultz, "Zur Physiologie der langsgestreiften (glatten) Muskeln," "Archiv f. Physiologie," suppl. volume, 1903, p. 1. See also Stewart, "Amer- ican Journal of Physiology," 4, 185, 1900. 56 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. is, to remain for long periods in a condition of greater or less con- traction. Doubtless this tonic contraction under normal relations is usually dependent upon stimulation received through the ner- vous system (neurogenic tonus), but the muscle, when completely isolated from the central nervous system, whether in or out of the body, continues to exhibit the phenomenon of tone to a remarkable degree. In most of the organs in which plain muscle occurs there are present also numerous nerve cells, and it is therefore still a question as to whether the tonic changes shown by this tissue, after separation of its extrinsic nerves, depend upon a property of the muscle itself (myogenic tonus) or upon their intrinsic nerve cells. Most observers adopt the former J id. 26. — Curve of simple contraction of plain muscle. The middle line is the time record, marking intervals of a second. The lowermost line indicates at the break the mo- ment of stimulation (short-lasting, tetanizing current). It will be seen that the latent period between beginning of stimulation and beginning of contraction is equal to about three seconds. view. The importance of this property of tone in the plain muscle tissues will be made fully apparent in the description of the physiology of the organs of circulation and digestion. Plain muscle may exhibit also the phenomenon of rhythmical activity — that is, under proper conditions it may contract and relax rhythmically like heart tissue.* Such movements have been observed and studied upon the plain muscle of the ureter, the bladder, the esophagus, stomach, and other portions of the alimentary canal, the spleen, the blood-vessels, etc. This property seems to be very unequally distributed among the different kinds ol plain muscle found in the same or different, animals, but this fact erves only to illustrate the point, already sufficiently empha- sized, thai grouping one kind of tissue e. f muscular metabolism, particularly the lactic acid. When t his substance accumulates in the muscle it may be carried * Ranke, "Tetanus," Leipzig, 1865. I Weitchardt, "Archiv f. Anat. u. Physiol, (physiol. Abth.)," 1905,219; also"Mttnchenermed. WoehenBchrift," L904, L905, L906. THE CHEMISTRY OF MUSCLE. 71 off in the blood and thus influence other organs. On such a supposition we may explain the fact, brought out by ergographic experiments, that marked exercise of one set of muscles, for example, those of the legs in walking or climbing, may diminish the amount of work obtainable from other unused muscles, such as those of the arms. So also the effect of muscular exercise upon the rate of the respiratory movements and upon the heart- rate is explained, as we shall see, in a similar way. It should be added that Lee,* confirming an older observation by Ranke, has published experiments which indicate that the first effect of the so-called fatigue substances is to increase the irritability of the muscle, while the later effect is to diminish the irritability or to suppress it altogether. In this initial favoring influence Lee finds an explanation of the phenomenon of Treppe (see p. 34). The theory of fatigue substances does not, however, explain all the phenomena, particularly the after-results. As was stated in describing the experiments made with the ergograph, a very short rest suffices to make the muscle again capable of lifting its load, but a very long interval of rest, two hours, may be required before the muscle is restored entirely to its normal condition. Such a long interval is probably not necessary* for the removal of the metabolic products, and we must recognize that a part of the fatigue is due to a using up of the material from which the energy is obtained. That is, during contraction the processes of disassimilation or catabolism are in excess of those of assimila- tion or anabolism, so that at the end of prolonged muscular activity the muscle contains a diminished supply of oxidizable or energy-yielding material. To supply this deficiency new food material must be received by the muscle. We must suppose, therefore, that two factors, accumulation of the products of metabolism and exhaustion of energy-yielding material, co- operate to produce the conditions actually observed; but the former of these, the formation of metabolic products, seems to be a protective mechanism that is especially adapted to save the muscle from complete exhaustion. In what way these products depress the irritability and contractility of the muscles is not known. Theories of Muscle Contraction. — It is universally admitted that the ultimate cause of the muscle contraction is the chemical change caused by the stimulus. While the nature of this chemical reaction is not known, it is admitted also that it consists in a process of splitting and oxidation whereby large and relatively unstable * For discussion and experiments, see Lee; Harvey Lectures, 1905-06, Philadelphia, 1906; also Journal of the American Medical Association, May 19, 1906, and American Journal of Physiology, 18, 267, 1907. 72 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. molecules are reduced to smaller and more stable ones, such as H,0 and the CO_, and lactic acid which we recognize among the products. This reaction is exothermic — that is, some of the chemical or internal energy of the complex compound is liberated as heat. Both of these results are so frequently observed in other chemical reactions that they call for no special comment in this case. The particular problem regarding the muscle is how this chemical reaction leads to the shortening of the muscle and thereby makes it do mechanical work. We must assume that there is some mechanism in the muscle by means of which the energy liberated during the chemical change is utilized in causing move- ment, somewhat in the same way as the heat enery de- veloped in a gas-engine is converted by a mechanism into mechanical movement, or the electrical energy in the coils of a motor is util- ized by a device to develop movement. Regarding the means used in the muscle to transform the original chem- ical or internal energy to me- chanical movement we have no or very little positive knowledge. Numerous theo- ries of a more or less specu- lative character have been proposed. It lias been sug- gested (Weber) that the mus- cular force is essentially due to the elasticity of tin; mus- cle. It is known Unit the elasticity of substances may change with conditions, and it is med that after stimulation the physical condition of the muscle is changed and thai the increased elastic attraction bc- 1 ween the particles gives it the form of the contracted muscle. According to others (Fick), the mechanical contraction is a direct re hIi of an increased chemical affinity, while others (Muller) find an explanation in supposed electrical charges upon the doubly refractive particles of the muscle, in consequence of which there are developed electrical attractions and repulsions at the Fin. '-7. — FnKelmann's artificial muscle. The artificial muscle is represented by the catgut r^t rintc, m. This is surrounded by a Coil of platinum wire, w, through which all electrical current may I"- sent. The catgut i atl:irlici| iii .-i lever, //, whose fulcrum ia at c. The catgut ia immersed in a beaker of water at 50 to 65° ('., and "stimulated" by the sudden increase in temperature oaui ed by lli<- i ■ ■■! i current through the coil. — (After Engelmann.) THE CHEMISTRY OF MUSCLE. 73 different poles. The most specific and comprehensible hypothesis advanced is that formulated by Engelmann.* This author has shown that all contractile tissues contain doubly refractive particles, that in the striped muscle fiber these particles are arranged in discs, — the dim bands, — with the singly refracting material forming the light bands on either side. During con- traction it has been shown that the material of this latter struc- ture is absorbed by the doubly refractive substance. Engelmann has shown, moreover, that dead substances, which contain doubly refractive particles and possess the property of imbibi- tion, such as catgut, when soaked with water will shorten upon heating and relax again upon cooling. His explanation of the mechanics of contraction in brief is that the chemical change brought about in the muscle liberates heat, and that the effect of this heat upon the adjacent doubly refractive particles is to make them imbibe the surrounding water. If we further suppose that these particles in the resting muscle are linear or prismatic in shape, then upon imbibing water they will tend to become spherical, causing thus a shortening in the long diameter and an increase in the cross diameter. The muscle, in other words, is an apparatus comparable, let us say, to a gas engine: each stimulus, like a spark, causes the physiological oxidation of a portion of the usable material in the muscle, and the heat thus produced acts upon the doubly refractive material as upon a piece of machin- ery and causes it to shorten by imbibition. Contraction, in a word, is a phenomenon of thermic imbibition. Engelmann has given an appearance of verisimilitude to this hypothesis by constructing an artificial muscle from a piece of violin string. The apparatus used is illustrated in Fig. 27. A catgut string (m) is surrounded by a coil of platinum wire (w) through which an electrical current may be sent. The object of this arrangement is to heat the catgut suddenly. The platinum coil should not actually touch the catgut. The catgut is attached to a lever, as shown in the figure. The catgut is thoroughly soaked by immersing it in a beaker of water and the temperature is then raised to 50° to 55° C. If then a current is turned into the coil the slight but somewhat rapid heating of the catgut will cause it to shorten, owing to the imbibition of more water. When the current is broken the catgut cools and relaxes slowly. Records may be obtained in this way which are altogether similar or identical with those given by a strip of plain muscle when stimulated (see Figs. 28 and 29). The model may be used to show the effect of temperature upon the extent and dura- * Engelmann, "Ueber den Ursprung der Muskelkraft, " Leipzig, 1893; see also "Pfliiger's Archiv," 7, 155, 1873; and "Archiv f. Physiologic, " 1907, 25. 74 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND XERYE. Fig. 28. — Curve of simple contraction obtained from an artificial muscle. The dura- tion of the stimulus (heating effect caused by the current) is shown by the break in the line beneath the curve. I ii' 29. [nutation of incomplete tetanus by the artificial muscle. The time and duration of the successive heatings are indicated by the breaks in the lower line. Each -iiir-li beating cause la s i is ra 1 1- eon i r.iH ion, and these contractions are summated as in the tetanic rijnir.icli'.ii of muscle. THE CHEMISTRY OF MUSCLE. 75 tion of the contractions, the effect of variations in strength of stimulus as expressed in the amount of current used, the sum- mation of successive stimuli, etc. Under all of these conditions it imitates closely the behavior of plain muscular tissue. Another somewhat similar explanation of the mechanics of contraction has been suggested by McDougall* and has obtained support from several observers. According to this view the change in form of the muscle is due to the passage of liquid from the surrounding sarcoplasm into the fibrillse (or sarcostyles) . This imbibition of liquid by the sarcostyles may be referred to the increase of osmotic pressure within them caused by the chemical changes following stimulation. The sarcostyles are divided transversely by the Krause membranes into sarcomeres, which have the form of elongated cylinders. By the absorption of water their form is changed to that of a flattened cylinder, hence the shortening. * McDougall, "Journal of Anatomy and Physiology," 1897, 31, 410, and 1898, 32, 187. See also Meigs, "Zeit. f. allgemeine Physiologie, " 1908, 8, 81. CHAPTER III. THE PHENOMENON OF CONDUCTION— PROPERTIES OF THE NERVE FIBER. Conduction. — When living matter is excited or stimulated in any way the excitation is not localized to the point acted upon, but is or may be propagated throughout its substance. This prop- erty of conducting a change that has been initiated by a stimulus applied locally is a general property of protoplasm, and is exhib- ited in a striking way by many of the simplest forms of life. A light touch, for instance, applied to a vorticella will cause a retrac- tion of its vibrating cilia and a shortening of its stalk. In the most specialized animals, such as the mammalia, this property of con- duction finds its greatest development in the nervous tissue, and indeed, especially in the axis cylinder processes of the nerve cells, the so-called nerve fibers. But the property is exhibited also to a greater or less extent by other tissues. When a muscular mass is stimulated at one point the excitation set up may be propagated not only through the substance of the cells or fibers directly affected, but from cell to cell for a considerable distance. In the heart tissue and in plain muscle it has been shown that a change of this sort may be conducted independently of the phenomenon of visible contraction. A stimulus applied to the venous end of a frog's heart, for instance, may, under certain conditions, be conducted through the auricular tissue without causing in it a visible change, and yet arouse a contraction in the ventricular muscle (Engelmann). Similarly, it can be shown that ciliary cells can convey a stimulus from cell to cell. A stimulus applied to one point of a field of ciliary epithelium may set up a change that is conveyed as a ciliary impulse to distant cells. The universality of this property of conduction in the simpler, less differentiated forms of life, and its presence in some form in many of the tissues of the higher forms would justify the as- sumption that the underlying change is essentially the same in all cases. Bui in nerve fibers this property has become special- ized to the highesl degree, and in this tissue it may be studied, therefore, with the greatest success and profit,. Structure of the Nerve Fiber. The peripheral nerve fiber, e find it in the nerve trunks and nerve plexuses of the body, may he either medullated or non-medullated. All the nerve fibers 76 THE PHENOMENON OF CONDUCTION. 77 that arise histologically from the nerve cells of the central nervous system proper — that is, the brain and cord and the outlying sensory ganglia of the cranial nerves and the posterior spinal roots — are medullated. These fibers contain a central core, the axis cylinder, which is usually regarded as an enormously elongated process of the nerve cell with which it is connected. The axis cylinder shows a differentiation into fibrils (neurofibrils) and interfibrillar sub- stance (neuroplasm). All of our evidence goes to show that the axis cylinder is the essential part of the nerve fiber so far as its property of conduction is concerned. It is further assumed that the neurofibrils in the axis cylinder form the conducting mech- anism rather than the interfibrillar substance. Surrounding the axis cylinder we have the medullary or myelin sheath, varying much in thickness in different fibers. This sheath is composed of peculiar material and is interrupted or divided into segments at cer- tain intervals, the so-called nodes of Ranvier. Outside the myelin there is a delicate elastic sheath comparable to the sarcolemma of the muscle fiber and designated as the neurilemma. Lying under the neurilemma are found nuclei, one for each internodal segment of the myelin, surrounded by a small amount of granular proto- plasm. The non-medullated fibers have no myelin sheath. They are to be considered as an axis cylinder process from a nerve cell, surrounded by or inclosed in a neurilemmal sheath. These fibers arise histologically from the nerve cells found in the outlying ganglia of the body, the ganglia of the sympathetic system and its appendages. The Function of the Myelin Sheath. — The myelin sheath of the cerebrospinal nerve fibers is a structure that is interesting and peculiar, both as regards its origin and its composition. Much speculation has been indulged in with regard to its function, but practically nothing that is certain can be said upon this point. It has been supposed by some to act as a sort of insulator, preventing contact between neighboring axis cylinders and thus insuring better conduction. But against this view it may be urged that we have no proof that the non-medullated fibers do not conduct equally as well. The view has some probability to it, however, for we must remember that the non-medullated fibers do not run in large nerve trunks that supply a number of different organs, and therefore in them a provision for isolated conduction is not so necessary. Moreover, in the medullated fibers the myelin sheath is lost toward its peripheral end after the nerve has entered the tissue to which it is to be distributed, indicating that its function is then no longer necessary. According to the older conceptions of the process of conduction in nerve fibers, not only anatomical but also physiological continuity is necessary. Mere contact of 78 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. living axis cylinders would not enable the nerve impulse to pass from one to the other. The newer views, included in the so-called neuron theory, assume that mere contact of living, entirely normal nerve substance does permit an excitatory change to pass from one to the other. So that it is not impossible that the myelin sheath may serve to prevent one axis cylinder from influencing the neighboring axis cylinders in a nerve trunk. Others have supposed that the myelin sheath serves as a source of nutrition to the inclosed axis cylinder, or as a regulator in some way of its metabolism. No fact is reported that would make this suggestion seem probable. In general, it is found that the myelin sheath is larger in those fibers that have the longest course; the size of the sheath, in fact, in- creases with that of the axis cylinder. It is known also that the medullated fibers in general are more irritable to artificial stimuli than the non-medullated ones, and that when induction shocks are employed the non-medullated fibers lose their irritability more rapidly at the point stimulated. None of these facts are sufficient, however, to indicate the probable function of the myelin. The embryological development of the sheath also fails to throw light on its physiological significance. For, while it is usually supposed that the axis cylinder itself is simply an outgrowth from the nerve cell, and the myelin sheath arises from separate mesoblastic cells which surround the axis cylinder, this view, so far as the myelin is con- cerned, is not beyond question, and the study of the process of regeneration of nerve fibers indicates that the actual production of myelin is controlled in some way by the functional axis cylinder. The axis cylinder outgrowths from the sympathetic nerve cells found in the ganglia of the sympathetic chain and in the peripheral ganglia generally of the body are usually non-medullated, although apparently this is not an invariable rule. In the birds all such fibers, on the contrary, are medullated. (Langley.*) Nothing is known as to the conditions that determine whether a nerve fiber process shall or shall not be surrounded by a myelin sheath. Chemistry of the Nerve Fiber. — Our knowledge of the chem- ist iv of the nerve fibers is very incomplete. The myelin sheath i composed largely of bodies such us lecithin and eholesterin, to which the general name of "lipoids" has been applied, the name having reference to their solubilities and not to their composition. By extraction of myelin with hot alcohol a complex phosphorus-containing substance known as protagon may he obtained in crystalline form. This substance is, how- ever, believed now to be a mixture rather than ;i definite chem- ical individual. The mosl important substances isolated from the myelin are lecithin, cholesterin, and the cerebrosides. * Langley, "Journal of Physiology, " :'><>, 221, L903; 20, 55, 1890. THE PHENOMENON OF CONDUCTION. 79 Lecithin (C4,HS4NP09) is a waxy hygroscopic yellowish sub- stance containing about 4 per cent, of phosphorus. When de- composed by the action of alkalies it yields as split products glycerophosphoric acid, a nitrogenous base, cholin (C5H15N02), and some of the higher fatty acids, such as oleic, palmitic, or stearic. It is probable that there are a number of different lecithines varying somewhat in their composition, for instance, in the character of the fatty acid contained in the molecule. These lecithin bodies or phosphatids, as they are sometimes named, are widely distributed in the tissues and liquids of the body, but are especially characteristic of the white matter of the nervous system. They combine easily with other substances, such as proteins, glucosides, etc., and it is probable that lecithin exists in some such combination in the myelin. The decom- position of the lecithin referred to above occurs in the body when nerves undergo degeneration. The presence of the fatty acid liberated under such circumstances is demonstrated by the well-known reaction with osmic acid used to detect degenerated nerve fibers, while the existence of cholin has been shown by Halliburton* in the liquids of the body not only after nerve- degeneration produced by experimental lesions, but in the case of degenerative diseases of the nervous system. Cholesterin (C27H460) is a white crystalline substance con- taining, as its formula shows, neither nitrogen nor phosphorus. It is widely distributed among the tissues of the body, and in an isomeric form phytocholesterin occurs also in plants. In the animal body it is especially abundant in the white matter of the nerves. The chemical nature of cholesterin has long been a matter of uncertainty, but recent work indicates that it belongs to the group of "terpenes" heretofore supposed to be confined to the plant kingdom. The fact that lecithin and cholesterin usually occur together has suggested that they may have some physiological connection. The chief fact indicating such a connection is the discovery that the hemolytic action of cobra venom on red blood corpuscles is made effective by activation of the toxin by lecithin (Kyes), while cholesterin, on the other hand, antagonizes this action of the lecithin. No application of this antagonistic relationship is possible at present in the case of the myelin sheath. Cerebrosides. — This name is given to a group of bodies con- taining nitrogen, but no phosphorus. In the myelin they are found in connection with and possibly in combination with the * Halliburton, "British Medical Journal," 1907, May 4 and 11. Also "Folia Neuro-Biologica, " 1907, i., 38, and " Biochemistry of Muscle and Nerve," Philadelphia, 1904. SO THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. lecithin. They belong to the group of glucosides, that is, on hydrolytic decomposition the}' give rise to a carbohydrate group, in this case galactose. Fatty acids and a nitrogen base also re- sult from this decomposition. The cerebroside material obtained from the white matter has been named specificially cerebrin or phrenosin, but little is known of its exact structure. Union of Nerve Fibers into Nerves or Nerve Trunks. — The assembling of nerve fibers into larger or smaller nerve trunks re- sembles histologically the combination of muscle fibers to form a muscle. Physiologically, however, there is no similarity. The various fibers in a muscle act together in a co-ordinated way as a physiological unit. On the other hand, the hundreds or thou- sands of nerve fibers found in a nerve may form groups which are entirely independent in their physiological activity. In the vagus nerve, for instance, we have nerve fibers running side by side, some of which supply the heart, some the muscles of the larynx, some the muscles of the stomach or intestines, some the glands of the stomach or pancreas, and so on. Nerves are, therefore, anatomical units simply, containing groups of fibers which have very different activities and which may function entirely independently of one another. Afferent and Efferent Nerve Fibers. — The older physiologists believed that one and the same nerve or nerve fiber might conduct sensory impulses toward the central nervous system or motor im- pulses from the central nervous system to the periphery. Bell and Magendie succeeded in establishing the great truth that a nerve fiber cannot be both motor and sensory. Since their time it has been recognized that we must divide the nerve fibers connected with the central nervous system into two great groups: the efferent fibers, which carry impulses outwardly from the nervous system to the peripheral tissues, and the afferent fibers, which carry their impulses inwardly, — that is, from the peripheral tissues to the nerve centers. Under normal conditions the afferent fibers are stimulated only at their endings in the peripheral tissues, in the skin, the mucous membranes, the sense organs, etc., while the efferent filters are stimulated only at their central origin,- that is, through the nerve cells from which they spring. The difference in the direction of conduction depends, therefore, on the anatomical fact that the efferent fibers have a stimulating mechanism at their central ends only, while the afferent fillers are adapted only for stimulation at their peripheral ends. Classification of Nerve Fibers.— In addition to this funda- mental separation we may subdivide peripheral nerve fibers into smaller groups, making use of either anatomical or physiological differences upon which to base a classification. For the purpose THE PHENOMENON OF CONDUCTION. 81 here in view a classification that is physiological as far as possible seems preferable. In the first place, experimental physiology has shown that the effect of the impulse conveyed by nerve fibers may be either exciting or inhibiting. That is, the tissue or the cell to which the impulse is carried may be thereby stimulated to ac- tivity, in which case the effect is excitatory, or, on the contrary, it may, if already in activity, be reduced to a condition of rest or lessened activity; the effect in this case is inhibitory. Many physiologists believe that one and the same nerve fiber may carry excitatory or inhibitory impulses, but in some cases at least we have positive proof that these functions are discharged by separate fibers. We may subdivide both the afferent and the efferent sys- tems into excitatory and inhibitory fibers. Each of these sub- groups again falls into smaller divisions according to the kind of activity it excites or inhibits. In the efferent system, for instance, the excitatory fibers may cause contraction or motion if they ter- minate in muscular tissue, or secretion if they terminate in glandu- lar tissue. For convenience of description each of the groups in turn may be further classified according to the kind of muscle in which it ends or the kind of glandular tissue. In the motor group we speak of vasomotor fibers in reference to those that end in the plain muscle of the walls of the blood-vessels; visceromotor fibers, those ending in the muscular tissue of the abdominal and thoracic viscera; pilomotor fibers, those ending in the muscles attached to the hair follicles. The classification that is suggested in tabular form below depends, therefore, on three principles: first, the direc- tion in which the impulse travels normally; second, whether this impulse excites or inhibits; third, the kind of action excited or inhibited, which in turn depends upon the kind of tissue in which the fibers end. Efferent Afferent Excitatory Inhibitory Excitatory Motor Secretory Inhibito-mo- tor Inhibito-se- cretorv Sensory 1 Reflex Inhibitory < Inhibito-re- flex f Motor. Vasomotor. Cardiomotor. Visceromotor. Pilomotor. Salivary. Gastric. Pancreatic. Sweat. Subdivisions corresponding to the varieties of mo- tor fibers above. Subdivisions corresponding to the varieties of se- cretory fibers above. Visual. Auditory. Olfactory. Gustatory. Pressu re. Temperature. Pain. Hunger. Thirst, etc. According to the efferent fibers affected. Inhibitory effects upon the conscious sensations are not demonstrated. The reflex fibers that cause unconscious reflexes are known to be inhibited in some cases at least. 82 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. That the final action of a peripheral nerve fiber is determined by the tissue in which it ends rather than by the nature of the nerve fiber itself or the nature of the impulse that it carries is indi- cated strongly by the regeneration experiments made by Langley.* For instance, the chorda tympani nerve contains fibers which cause a dilatation in the blood-vessels of the submaxillary gland, while the cervical sympathetic contains fibers which cause a constriction of the vessels in the same gland. If the lingual nerve (containing the chorda tympani fibers) is divided and the central end is sutured to the peripheral end of the severed cervical sympathetic, the chorda fibers will grow along the paths of the old constrictor fibers of the sympathetic. If time is given for regeneration to take place, stimulation of the chorda now causes a constriction in the vessels. The experiment can also be reversed. That is, by suturing the central end of the cervical sympathetic to the peripheral end of the divided lingual the fibers of the former grow along the paths of the old dilator fibers, and after regeneration has taken place stimulation of the sympathetic causes dilatation of the blood- vessels in the gland. These results are particularly instructive, as vasoconstriction is an example of the excitatory effect of the nerve impulse, being the result of a contraction of the circular muscles in the vessels, while vasodilatation is an example of inhibitory action, being due to an inhibition of the contraction of the same muscles. Yet obviously these two opposite effects are determined not by the nature of the nerve fibers, but by their place or mode of ending in the gland. Separation of the Afferent and Efferent Fibers in the Roots of the Spinal Nerves. — According to the Bell-Magendie discovery, the motor fibers to the voluntary muscles emerge from the spinal cord in the anterior roots, while the fibers that give rise to sensa- tions enter the cord through the posterior roots. These facts have been demonstrated beyond all doubt. Magendie discovered an apparent exception in the phenomenon of recurrent sensibility. When the anterior root is severed and its peripheral end is stimu- lated only motor effects should be obtained. Magendie observed, however, upon dogs that in certain cases the animals showed signs of pain. This apparent exception to the general rule was after- ward explained satisfactorily. It was shown (hat the fibers in question do not really belong to the anterior root, — that is, they do not emerge from the cord with the roof fibers; they are, in fact, iry fibers for the meningeal membranes of the cord which are on their way to the posterior roots and which enter the cord with the fibers of the latter. Since the work of Bell and Magendie it has been a quest ion whet her t heir law applies to all afferent and * Langley, "Journal of Physiology," 23, 240, L898; ibid., 30, 439, L904; " Proceedings Royal Society,'' 7:;, L904, THE PHENOMENON OF CONDUCTION. 83 efferent fibers and not simply to the motor and sensory fibers proper. The experimental evidence upon this point, as far as the mammals are concerned, has accumulated slowly. Various authors have shown that stimulation of the anterior roots of certain spinal nerves may cause a constriction of the blood-vessels, an erection of the hairs (stimulation of the pilomotor fibers), a secretion of sweat, and so on, while stimulation of the posterior roots in the same regions is without effect upon these peripheral tissues. One apparent excep- tion, however, has been noted. A number of observers have found that stimulation of the peripheral end of the divided posterioi roots (fifth lumbar to first sacral) causes a vascular dilatation in the hind limb. The matter has been particularly investigated by Bayliss,* who gives undoubted proof of the general fact. At the same time he shows that the fibers in question are not efferent fibers from the cord passing out by the posterior instead of the an- terior roots. This is shown by the fact that they do not degenerate when the root is cut between the ganglion and the cord, as they should do if they originated from cells in the cord. Bayliss's own explanation of this curious fact is that the fibers in question are ordinary afferent fibers, but that they are capable of a double ac- tion: they can convey sensory impulses from the blood-vessels to the cord according to the usual type of sensory fibers, but they can also convey efferent impulses, antidromic impulses as he desig- nates them, to the muscles of the blood-vessels. In other words, for this special set of fibers he attempts to re-establish the view held by physiologists before the time of Bell, — namely, that one and the same fiber transmits normally both afferent and efferent impulses. An exception so peculiar as this to an otherwise general rule cannot be accepted without hesitation. It is possible that future work may give an explanation less opposed to current views than that offered by Bayliss. Cells of Origin of the Anterior and Posterior Root Fibers.— The efferent fibers of the anterior root arise as axons or axis cylinder processes from nerve cells in the gray matter of the cord at or near the exit of the root. The motor fibers to the voluntary muscles arise from the large cells of the anterior horn of gray matter; the fibers to the plain muscle and glands, autonomic fibers according to Langley's nomenclature, take their origin from spindle-shaped nerve cells lying in the so-called lateral horn of the gray matter.f According to the accepted belief regarding the nutrition of nerve fibers, an}*- section or lesion involving these portions of the gray mat- ter or the anterior root will be followed by a complete degeneration of the efferent fibers. In the case of the fibers to the voluntary muscles this degeneration will extend to the muscles and include * Bayliss, "Journal of Physiology," 26, 173, 1901, and 28, 276, 1902. t Herring, "Journal of Physiology,''' 29, 2S2, 1903. 84 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. the end-plates. In the case of the autonomic fibers the degenera- tion will extend to the peripheral ganglia in which they terminate, involving, therefore, the whole extent of what is called the pre- ganglionic fiber (see the chapter on the autonomic nerves and the sympathetic system). The posterior root, fibers have their origin in the nerve cells contained in the posterior root ganglia. These cells are unipolar, the single process given off being an axis cylinder process or axon. It divides into two branches, one passing into the cord by way of the posterior root, the other toward the periph- eral tissues in the corresponding spinal nerve in which they form the peripheral sensory nerve fibers. It follows that a section or lesion of the posterior root will result in a degeneration of the branch entering the cord, this branch having been cut off from its nutri- tive relationship with its cells of origin. The degeneration will in- volve the entire length of the branch and its collaterals to their terminations among the dendrites of other spinal or bulbar neurons (see the chapter on the spinal cord). After a lesion of this sort the stump of the posterior root that remains in connection with the posterior root ganglion maintains its normal structure. On the other hand, a section or lesion involving the spinal nerve will be followed by a degeneration of all the fibers, efferent and afferent, lying to the peripheral side of the lesion, since these fibers are cut off from connection with their cells of origin, while the fibers in the central stump of the divided nerve will retain their normal structure. Afferent and Efferent Fibers in the Cranial Nerves. — The first and second cranial nerves, the olfactory and the optic, contain onl)r afferent fibers, which arise in the former nerve from the olfac- tory epithelium in the nasal cavity, in the latter from the nerve cells in the retina. The third, fourth, and sixth nerves contain only efferent fibers which arise from the nerve cells constituting their nuclei of origin in the midbrain and pons. The fifth nerve resembles the spinal nerves in that it has two roots, one containing afferent and the other efferent fibers. The efferent fibers, consti- tuting the small root, arise from nerve cells in the pons and mid- brain, the afferent fibers arise from the nerve cells in the Gasserian ganglion. This ganglion, being a sensory ganglion, is constituted like the posterior root, ganglia. Its nerve cells give off a single process which divides in T, one branch passing into the brain by way of the large root, while the other passes to the peripheral tissues as a sensory fiber of the fifth nerve. The seventh nerve may also be homologized with a spinal nerve. The facial nerve proper consists of only efferent fibers, which arise from nerve cells constituting its nucleus of origin in the pons. The geniculate ganglion, attached to this nerve shortly after its emergence, is similar in structure to the I rasseriarj or a posterior root ganglion. Its nerve cells send off processes which divide in T and constitute afferent fibers in the THE PHENOMENON OF CONDUCTION. 85 so-called nervus intermedius or nerve of Wrisberg. The eighth nerve consists only of afferent fibers which arise from the nerve cells in the spiral ganglion of the cochlea, cochlear branch, and from those constituting the vestibular or Scarpa's ganglion, the vestibu- lar branch. Both of these ganglia are sensory, resembling the posterior root ganglia in structure. The ninth nerve is also mixed, the efferent fibers arising from the motor nucleus in the medulla, while the sensory fibers arise in the superior and petrosal ganglia found on the nerve at its emergence from the skull. The tenth is a mixed nerve, its efferent fibers arising in motor nuclei in the me- dulla, the afferent fibers in the nerve cells of the ganglia lying upon the trunk of the nerve at its exit from the skull (ganglion jugulare and nodosum). The eleventh and twelfth cranial nerves contain only efferent fibers that arise from motor nuclei in the medulla. It will be seen from these brief statements that in all the nerve trunks of the central nervous system — that is, the spinal and the cranial nerves — the cells of origin of the efferent fibers lie within the gray matter of the brain or cord, while the cells of origin of the afferent fibers lie in sensory ganglia outside the central nervous system, — namely, in the posterior root ganglia for the spinai nerves, in the ganglion semilunare (Gasseri), the g. geniculi, the g. spirale, the g. vestibulare, the g. superius and g. petrosum of the glossopharyngeal, and the g. jugulare and g. nodosum of the vagus. These various sensory ganglia attached to the cranial nerves corre- spond essentially in their structure and physiology with the posterior root ganglia of the spinal nerves. Independent Irritability of Nerve Fibers. — Although the nerve fibers under normal conditions are stimulated only at their ends, the efferent fibers at the central end, the afferent at the peripheral end, yet any nerve fiber may be stimulated by artificial means at any point in its course. Artificial stimuli capable of affecting the nerve fiber — that is, capable of generating in it a nerve impulse which then propagates itself along the fiber — may be divided into the following groups : 1. Chemical stimuli. Various chemical reagents, when applied directly to a nerve trunk, excite the nerve fibers. Such reagents are concentrated solutions of the neutral salts of the alkalies, acids, alkalies, glycerin, etc. This method of stimulation is not, however, of much practical value in experimental work, since it is difficult or impossible to control the reaction. 2. Mechanical stimuli. A blow or pressure or a mechanical in- jury of any kind applied to a nerve trunk also excites the fibers. This method of stimulating the fibers is also difficult to control and has had, therefore, a limited application in experimental work. The mechanical stimulus is essentially a pressure stimulus, and the 86 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. difficult}- lies in controlling this pressure so that it shall not actually destroy the nerve fiber by rupturing the delicate axis cylinder. Various instruments have been devised by means of which light blows may be given to the nerve, sufficient to arouse an impulse, but insufficient to permanently injure the fibers. The results ob- tained by this method have been very valuable in physiology as con- trols for the experiments made by the usual method of electrical stimulation. It may be mentioned also that under certain condi- tions— for instance, at one stage in the regeneration of injured nerve fibers mechanical stimuli may be more effective than electrical, that is, may stimulate the nerve fiber when electrical stimuli totally fail to do so. 3. Thermal stimuli. A sudden change in temperature may stimulate the nerve fibers. This method of stimulation is very ineffective for motor fibers, only very extreme and sudden changes, such as may be obtained by applying a heated wire directly to the nerve trunk, are capable of so stimulating them as to produce a muscular contraction. On the other hand, the sensory nerve fibers are quite sensitive to changes of temperature. If a nerve trunk in a man or animal is suddenly cooled, or especially if it is suddenly heated to 60° to 70° C, violent pain results from the stimulation of the sensory fibers in the trunk, while the motor fibers are apparently not acted upon. We have in this fact one of several differences in reaction between motor and sensory fibers which have been noted from time to time, and which seem to Fig. .'JO. — Stimulating (catheter) electrodes for nerves: b. Binding posts for attachment of wires from the secondary coil; «, insulating sheath of hard rubber; p, platinum points laid upon the nerve. indicate thai there is some important difference in structure or composition between them. 4. Electrical stimuli. Some form of the electrical current is be- yond quest ion the most effective and convenient means of stimulat- ing nerve fibers. We may employ either the galvanic current — that is, the current taken directly from a battery — or the induced current from the secondary coil of an induction apparatus or the so-called static electricity from a Leyden jar or other source. In most experi- mental work the induced current is used. The terminal wires from the secondary coil are connected usually with platinum wires im- bedded in hard rubber, forming what is known as a stimulating elec- trode. (See Fig. 30.) By this means the platinum ends which nov THE PHENOMENON OF CONDUCTION. 87 form the electrodes, anode and cathode, can be placed close together upon the nerve trunk, and the induced current passing from one to the other through a short stretch of the nerve sets up at that point nerve impulses which then propagate themselves along the nerve fibers. The induction current is convenient because of its intensity, which overcomes the great resistance offered by the moist tissue ; be- cause of its very brief duration, in consequence of which it acts as a sharp, quick, single stimulus or shock, and because of the great ease with which it may be varied as to rate and as to intensity. Each time that the battery current in the primary coil is made or broken there is an induction current established in the secondary coil, and if the nerve is on the electrode the current passes through it and stimulates it. This induced current is, however, extremely short, and alternates in direction, passing in one direction when the primary current is made and in the opposite direction when it is broken. The induced current set up by the making of the battery- current in the primary coil we designate as the making shock, that set up by the breaking of the current in the primary as the breaking shock. On account of the very brief duration of the induced cur- rent it is difficult to distinguish between the effects of its opening and closing. The Stimulation of the Nerve by the Galvanic Current. — When however, we employ the galvanic current, taken directly from a bat- tery, as a stimulus, we can, of course, allow the current to pass through the nerve as long as we please and can thus study the effect of the closing of the current as distinguished from that of the open- ing, or the effect of duration or direction of the current, etc. Du Bois-ReymonaV s Law of Stim- ulation.— When a galvanic current is led into a motor nerve it is found, as a rule, that with all moderate strengths of currents there is a stimulus to the nerve at the moment it is closed, the making or closing stimulus, and another when the current is broken, the breaking or opening stimulus, while during the passage of the current through the nerve no stimulation takes place: the muscle remains relaxed. We may express this fact by saying that the motor nerve fibers are stimulated b}^ the mak- Fig. 31. — Schema of the arrange- ment of apparatus for stimulating the nerve by a galvanic current: b. The battery; k, the key for opening and closing the circuit ; c, the commutator for reversing the direction of the cur- rent; + the anode or positive pole; — the cathode or negative pole. 88 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AXD NERVE. ing and the breaking of the current or by any sudden change in its intensity, but remain unstimulated during the passage of cur- rents whose intensity does not vary. The Anodal and Cathodal Stimuli. — It has been shown quite con- clusively that the nerve impulse started bj' the making of the current arises at the cathode, while that at the breaking of the current begins at the anode, or, in other words, the making shock or stimulus is cathodal, while the breaking stimulus is anodal. This fact is true for muscle as well as nerve, and possibly for all irritable tissues capable of stimulation by the galvanic current. This important generalization may be demonstrated for motor nerves by separating the anode and cathode as far as possible and re- cording the latent period for the contractions caused respect- ively by the making and the breaking of the current in the nerve. If the cathode is nearer to the muscle the latent period of the mak- ing contraction of the muscle will be shorter than that of the break- ing contraction by a time equal to that necessary for a nerve impulse to travel the distance between anode and cathode. If the position of the electrodes is reversed the latent period of the making con- traction will be correspondingly longer than that of the breaking contraction. It is very evident from these facts that when a current is passed into a nerve or muscle the changes at the two poles are different, as shown by the differences in reactions and properties of the nerve at these points. Bethe has shown that a difference may be demonstrated even by histological means. After the passage of a current through a nerve for some time the axis cylinders stain more deeply than normal at the cathode with cer- tain dyes (toluidin blue), while at the anode they stain less deeply. Electrotonus. — The altered physiological condition of the nerve at the poles during the passage of the galvanic current is designated as electrotonus, the condition round the anode being known as anelectrotonus, thai round the cathode as catelectrotonus. Elec- i n.t onus expresses itself as a change in the electrical condition of the nerve which gives rise to currents known as the electrotonic currents, a brief description of these currents will he given in the next chapter, — and also by a change in irritability and con- ductivity. The latter changes were first carefully investigated by Pflliger, who showed that when 1 he galvanic current, or, as it is usually called in this connection, the polarizing current, is not too strong there is ;ui increase in irritability and conductivity in the neighborhood of the cathode, the so-called catelectrotonic increase of irritability, while in the region of the anode there is an anelec- trotonic decrease in irritability and conductivity. These opposite variations in the state of the nerve are represented in the accom- panying diagram. Between the two poles that is, in the intrapolar THE PHENOMENON OF CONDUCTION. 89 region — there is, of course, an indifferent point, on one side of which the irritability of the nerve is above normal and on the other side below normal. The position of this indifferent point shifts toward the cathode as the strength of the polarizing current is increased. In other words, as the current increases the anelectrotonus spreads more rapidly and becomes more intense, and the conductivity in this region soon becomes so depressed as to block entirely the passage of a nerve impulse through it. The changes on the cathodal side are not so constant nor so distinct. Later observers * have shown, in fact, that if the polarizing current is continued for some time the heightened irritability at the cathode soon diminishes and sinks below normal, so that in fact at the cathode as well as at the anode the irritability may be lost entirely. If the polarizing current is very strong this depressed irritability at the cathode comes on practically at once. Moreover, when a strong current that has been passing through a nerve is broken the condition of depressed irritability at the cathode persists for some time after the opening of the current. Pflilger's Law of Stimulation. — It was said above that when a galvanic current is passed into a nerve there is a stimulus (catho- dal) at the making of the current and another stimulus (anodal) Fig. 32. — Electrotcnic alterations of irritability caused by weak, medium, and strong battery currents: A and B indicate the points of application of the electrodes to the nerve, A being the anode, B the cathode. The horizontal line represents the nerve at normal irri- tability; the curved lines illustrate how the irritability is altered at different parts of the nerve with currents of different strengths. Curve yl shows the effect of a weak current, the part below the line indicating decreased, and that above the line increased irritability; at a:1 the curve crosses the line, this being the indifferent point at which the catelectrotonic effects are compensated for by anelectrotonic effects; y- gives the effect of a stronger current, and Vs, of a still stronger current. As the strength of the current is increased the effect becomes greater and extends farther into the extrapolar regions. In the intrapolar region the in- different point is seen to advance, with increasing strengths of current, from the anode toward the cathode. — {Lombard.) at the breaking of the current. This statement is true, however, only for a certain range of currents. Of the two stimuli, the making or cathodal stimulus is the stronger, and it follows, therefore, *Werigo, "Pflilger's Archiv," 84, 547, 1901. See Biedermann, " Elec- trophysiology," translated by Welby, vol. ii, p. 140. 90 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AXD NERVE. that when the strength of the current is diminished there will come a certain point at which the anodal stimulus will drop out. With weak currents there is then a stimulus only at the make. On the other hand, when very strong currents are used the stimuli that act at the two poles set up nerve impulses whose passage to the muscle may be blocked by the depressed conductivity caused by the electro- tonic changes. "Whether or not the stimulus will be effective in causing a contraction in the attached muscle will depend naturally on the relative positions of the electrodes, — that is, on the direction of the current in the nerve. In describing the effect of these strong currents we must distinguish between what are called ascending and descending currents. Ascending currents are those in which the direction of the current in the nerve is away from the muscle, a position of the poles, therefore, in which the anode is closer to the muscle. In descending currents the positions are reversed. Pfliiger's law of contraction or of stimulation takes account of the effect of extreme variations in the strength of the current and is usually expressed in tabular form as follows: The letter C indicates that the nerve is stimulated and causes a contraction in the attached muscle, and O indicates a failure in the stimulation (weak currents) or a failure in the nerve impulse to reach the muscle owing to blocking (strong currents). Fig. 33. — Schema to Hhow the arrangement of apparatus for an ascending and a descending current: PI, ascending; D, descending. Ascending Current. Descending Current. Making, Brcakinu. Making. Breaking, Very weak currents .. C 0 C O Moderate " . . . .C C C C Very strong " 0 C C O The effects obtained with the strong currents are readily under- stood if we bear in mind the fads stated above regarding cleetro- tonus. When the current is ascending the stimulus on making Mails from the cathode, but cannot reach the muscle because it is blocked by a region of anelectrotonus in which the conduc- THE PHENOMENON OF CONDUCTION. 91 tivity is depressed. The stimulus on breaking takes place at the anode and the impulse encounters no resistance in its passage to the muscle. With the descending current the cathode lies next to the muscle and the making or cathodal stimulus of course causes a contraction. On breaking, however, the impulse that is started from the anode is blocked by the depressed irritability in the cathodal region, which, as has been said, comes on promptly with strong currents and persists for a time after the current is broken. The Opening and the Closing Tetanus. — While the du Bois-Reymond law stated above expresses the facts as usually observed upon a nerve-muscle preparation, there are a number of observations which indicate that the excitation at the anode and the cathode during the passage of a current may give rise to a series of stimuli instead of a single stimulus. Thus with sensory nerves it is well known that the stimulation, as judged by the sensations aroused, continues while the current is passing instead of being limited to the moment of making or of breaking of the current. In this respect, as in stimulation by high temperatures, the sensory fibers differ apparently from the motor. When a galvanic current is passed through the ulnar nerve at the elbow sensations are felt during the entire time of passage of the current. But in an ordinary nerve-muscle preparation it is also fre- quently observed that at the moment of opening the current a tetanic con- traction, persisting for some time, is obtained instead of a single twitch. This phenomenon is known as the opening tetanus or Ritter's tetanus, and Pfluger has shown that the continuous excitation proceeds from the anode, since in the case of a descending current division of the nerve in the intrapolar region brings the muscle to rest. In the same way it frequently happens that upon closing the current through a nerve the muscle, instead of giving a twitch, goes into a persistent tetanic contraction. The tetanus in this case is designated as the closing or Pfliiger's tetanus. Both of these phenomena are observed, especially when the irritability of the nerve is for any reason greater than normal. It seems probable, therefore, to many observers that the excitation at the cathode persists in reality during the passage of the current even in motor fibers, although ordinarily the excitation makes itself felt upon the muscles only at the moment of closure ; the excitations during the passage of. the current being either too weak to affect the muscle or the condition of the nerve being such as to prevent their conduction to the muscle. Tt should be added that the opening and the closing tetanus may be observed also in a muscle when the galvanic current is passed through it. Stimulation of the Nerves in Man. — For therapeutic as well as diagnostic and experimental purposes it often becomes desirable to stimulate the nerves, particularly the motor nerves, in man. We may use for this purpose either the induced (faradic, alternat- ing) current or the direct battery current (galvanic or continuous current) . In such cases the electrodes cannot be applied, of course, directly to the nerve; it becomes necessary to stimulate through the skin, and the so-called unipolar method is employed. The unipolar method consists in placing one electrode, the active or stimulating electrode, over the nerve at the point which it is desired to stimulate, while the other electrode, the inactive or indifferent electrode, is applied to the skin at some more or less remote part, usually at the back of the neck. The indifferent electrode is made large enough to cover several square centimeters of the skin, and one 92 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. may conceive the threads of current passing from it into the moist tissues of the body and thence to the active electrode. As the threads of current condense to this latter electrode they pass through the motor nerve which lies under it, and if sufficiently intense -will stimulate the nerve. The arrangement is represented in the accom- panying schema (Fig. 34), showing the disposition of the electrodes for stimulating the median nerve. At the indifferent electrode the sensory nerves of the skin are of course stimulated, but no motor response is obtained, as no motor nerve lies immediately under the skin. Moreover the large size of this electrode tends to diffuse the current and thus reduce its effectiveness in stimulating. The active or stimulating electrode is small in size, particularly when induction currents are employed, so that the current may be condensed and thus gain in effectiveness. The dry surface of the skin is a poor conductor of the electrical current, and to reduce the resistance at the points at which the electrodes come in contact with the skin each is Fig. 34.— Schema to tihaw the unipolar method of stimulation in man. The anode, +, IH r'rj.i r- ented aa t i • »; Stimulating pole, applied over t lie median nerve. The cathode, — , ia the indifferent pole. t covered with cotton or chamois skin kept moistened with a dilute saline solution. Motor Points. — By means of the unipolar method nearly every voluntary muscle of the body may be stimulated separately. All THE PHENOMENON OF CONDUCTION. 93 that is necessary, when the induced current is used, is to bring the active electrode as nearly as possible over the spot at which the muscle receives its motor branch. A diagram showing these motor points for the arm is given in Fig. 35. In the same way the nerves of the brachial plexus and other nerve trunks may be stimulated very readily through the skin. When the induction current is used no distinction is made between the cathodic and anodic effects. When, however, the battery current is employed U. flexor carpi ulnaris tferv. musculacutaneuM M. biceps brachil M. brach. interacts Tvf, flexor digitor. suljliro. M. abductor pollic. brer. M. opponens pollicis M. flex. poll. brev. M. adductor pollic, brtT- Fig. 35. — Motor points in upper extremity. one may make the stimulating electrode either anode or cathode, and under these circumstances a marked difference is observed in the strength of the current that it is necessary to use to get a response. With the battery or galvanic current, in fact, one may distinguish four stimuli, the closing and the open- ing shock when the stimulating electrode is cathode and the closing and the opening shock when it is anode. The con- tractions resulting from these four stimuli are designated usually as follows: The cathoclol closing contraction, C C C; the cathodal 94 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. opening contraction, C 0 C; the anodal closing contraction, ACC; and the anodal opening contraction, A O C. If the minimal amount of current necessary to give each of these contractions is measured in milliamperes by means of a suitable ammeter, it will be found that the four stimuli are of different efficiencies. The usual relationship is expressed by the sequence C C C > ACC > A 0 C >COC, although this sequence is subject to some individual variation. Certain pathological or traumatic lesions that cause the degeneration of the nerves may be revealed by the use of these methods of stimulation. The nerve trunk under such circumstances fails to respond to either form of stimulus, induced or galvanic. The muscle, on the other hand, while it fails to respond to induction shocks, is stimulated by the galvanic current and, indeed, may show an increased irritability toward this form of stimulus, although the contractions are more sluggish in character than in a muscle with a normal nerve supply. Certain qualitative changes in the reaction of the muscle to the galvanic current may also be noticed, for instance, the A C C is sometimes obtained with less current than the C C C. This qualitative and quantitative change in reaction to the galvanic current, and the loss of irritability to the induced cur- rent, constitute what is known as the reaction of degeneration. (0$~ —L^fC yd- j a. ia. Fig. 38. — Two schemata to show the relation between the physical and the physio- logical electrodes or pole . Kach schema represents the forearm with the median nerve, M . In / the tim illation electrode is I lie cathode; the tine: ids of current which have started from the anode (the inditfcient electrode) placed elsewhere, conver«e to this pole. Where these thread eater the nerve we have a series of physiological anodes, a ; where they leave, a erie of physiological cathode , c. In // the stimulating electrode is the anode. The thread of current leave thi pole to traverse the body toward the indifferent electrode (cathode). Where they enter and leave the nerve we have, as in the first case, physio- logical anode and cathode-., now, however, on the opposite sides of the nerve. Distinction between Physical and Physiological Poles. — The facte stated above 3eem to show, at first sight, that by the unipolar method we may obtain both an opening and a closing shock at cit Iter the cathode or anode, a result which is in THE PHENOMENON OF CONTRACTION. 95 apparent contradiction to the general law that the making or closing stimulus occurs only at the cathode and the breaking or opening stimulus only at the anode. This apparent contra- diction is readily explained when we remember that in the unipolar method the active electrode rests upon the skin over the nerve, and that the threads of current radiating from this point enter the nerve at one point and leave it at another. Evidently, therefore, so far as the nerve is concerned, there will be an anode where the current is considered as entering the nerve and a cathode where it leaves it, so that under the active electrode, whether this is physically an anode or cathode, there will be, as regards the nerve, a series of what may be called physiological cathodes and anodes. The closing shock arises at these cathodes, the opening shock at the anodes. The position of the series of anodes and cathodes will vary according as the active electrode is an anode or cathode, as is indicated in the accompanying diagram (Fig. 36). CHAPTER IV. THE ELECTRICAL PHENOMENA SHOWN BY NERVE AND MUSCLE. The Demarcation Current. — Our definite knowledge of the electrical properties of living tissue began with the celebrated in- vestigations of du Bois-Reymond* (1843). When a muscle or nerve is removed from the body, and, in the case of the muscle, when one tendinous end is cut off, it is found that the cut end has an electrical potential differing from that of the uninjured longi- tudinal surface of the preparation. Following the usual nomen- clature, the cut end is electronegative as regards the longitudinal surface. If, therefore, the longitudinal surface is connected by a conductor with the cut surface a current will flow from the former to the latter, as is indicated in the accompanying diagram, + Fig. 37. — Schema .showing the course of the demarcation current in an excised nerve, when a point on the longitudinal and one on the cut surface are united by a conductor. While the direction of the current through the conductor con- necting the two points is from the longitudinal to the cut surface the current may be considered as being completed in the opposite direction within the substance of the muscle or nerve, as shown in the diagram. We may, in fact, consider an excised nerve or muscle as a battery, the cut end representing the zinc plate and the longitudinal surface the copper plate. Within the battery the direction of the current is from zinc to copper, from cut end to longitudinal surface; outside the battery the direction is from copper to zinc, from longitudinal to cut surface. If two wires are connected with the muscle or nerve the end of the one attached to the longitudinal surface will represent the positive pole or anode, the end of the one attached to the cut end will represent the cathode * "Untersuchungen liber thierische Elektricitfit," Bhow tin- relation <>f the thread to the magnets in the string- galvanometei l I, The delicate thread of silvered quartz or of platinum, stretched between 1 1 ■ ' ■ polai piece {PP) of an electromagnet, When a current passes through .1 A , the thread b movement. The ends of the magnets are pierced by holes, Been in /'i, through which the movements "I the thread may !><■ watched by means of a microscope! or he pro- jected upon ,-i photographic plate. (After Einthoven.) metal poles of the magnel are pierced by boles, so that the thread may be illuminated by an eled ric light < Nernsl la nip) from one i ide, and on the other the Bhadow of the thread may be thrown upon a screen after being magnified by a microscope (see Fig. 42). With this arrangement the thread shows a ELECTRICAL PHENOMENA. 101 --3 lateral movement whenever a current is passed through it. The instrument may be made of great delicacy so as to detect very minute currents, and, moreover, it has the very great advantage of responding accurately to rapid changes in potential. If the shadow of the thread is allowed to fall upon sensitized paper properly adjusted upon a rotating surf ace, its movements may be photographed and a permanent record be thus obtained (see Fig. 22 for an example of such a photographic record showing the electrical changes in a contracting muscle) . The Capillary Electrometer. — The principle of the construction of the capillary electrometer is illustrated in Fig. 41. A glass tube, a, is drawn out at one end into a very fine capillary, the end of which dips into some diluted sulphuric acid contained in the vessel (/). At the bottom of this vessel is a layer of mercury connecting with a wire, g, fused into the glass vessel. The tube a is partially filled with redistilled mercury, which pene- trates for a short distance into the capillary. By means of pressure applied from above c, the mercury can be forced through the capillary. Then by diminishing the pressure the mercury can be brought back into the capillary a certain distance, drawing after it some of the dilute sulphuric acid. The mercury in tube a is connected with the other pole of the battery by a wire fused into its wall and dipping into the mercury. By regulating the pressure on the mercury the point of contact be- tween the thread of mercury and the sulphuric acid in the capillary, d, can be brought to any desired position. An equilibrium is then established which will remain constant as long as the conditions are not changed. If now the circuit from a battery or other source of electricity — for example, the excised nerve or muscle — is closed, the current entering by wire g, if this represents the anode, traverses the sulphuric acid and mercury in the capillary and returns by the wire h. At the moment of the establishment of the current the equilibrium of forces that holds the mer- cury at a certain point in the capillary is disturbed, the end of the mercury thread moves upward with the current for a certain distance, depending on the strength of the current and the delicacy of the capillary. If the current be passed in the opposite direction the mercury will move downward a certain distance. The meniscus of contact moves up or down with the direc- tion of the current, owing, it is supposed, to a change in the surface tension at this point. The capillary tube as used for physiological purposes is too small for the movements of the mercury to be detected with the eye. It is necessary to magnify it either with a microscope or a projection lantern. Ordinarily the electrometer is so made that it can be placed upon the stage of the microscope and the capillaries be brought into focus at the meniscus, as shown in d, Fig. 41. By means of proper apparatus the movement can be photographed and thus a permanent record be obtained of the direc- tion and extent of movement of the mercury. Non-polarizable Electrodes. — In connecting a muscle or nerve to an elec- trometer or galvanometer it is necessary that the leading off electrodes — that is, the point of contact between the wires and the muscle or nerve — shall be iso-electrical and non-polarizable. By iso-electrical is meant that the two electrodes shall have the same electrical potential, and it is obvious that the leading off electrodes must fulfil this condition approximately at least, since otherwise the current obtained from the muscle or nerve could not be attrib- uted to differences in potential in the tissue itself; it would be shown by any other moist conductor connecting the two electrodes. Two clean platinum electrodes would fulfil this condition. A more serious difficulty is found in Fig. 44.— To show the structure of a non- polarizable electrode: 1, The pad of kaolin or filter paper moistened with physiological sa- line (NaCl, 0.7 per cent.) (this is placed on the tissue) ; 2, the sat- urated solution of zinc sulphate; (3) the_ bar of amalgamated zinc. 102 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. the polarization of metallic electrodes. Whenever a metal conductor and a liquid conductor come into contact there is apt to be polarization. What takes place may be represented by the following diagram, in which a current is supposed to be passing + + Na + Na + Na + Na A CI CI CI CI ~~ — — between the poles A and C through a solution of sodium chlorid. During the passage of the current the cations, Na, with their positive charges move toward the cathode; at the cathode the free sodium ion acts upon the water, HHO, forming NaOH and liberating hydrogen, which gives its charge to the cathode and accumulates upon it in the form of gas. The anions, CI, with their negative charges move toward the anode; there the chlorin acts upon the water, forming HC1 and liberating oxygen. In conse- quences of these chemical actions at the poles an electromotive force is de- veloped at the cathode which diminishes the current passing from A to C. It is obvious that in quantitative studies of the electrical currents of animal tissues polarization will destroy the accuracy of the results; the demarcation current will show a diminution due not to changes in the nerve, but to physi- cochemical changes at the leading off electrodes. To prevent polarization du Bois-Reymond devised the non-polarizable electrodes consisting of zinc terminals immersed in zinc sulphate. Theoretically any metal in a solution of one of its salts may be used, but experience shows that the zinc-zinc sulphate electrode is most nearly perfect. Each electrode where it comes into contact with the tissue is made of one of these combinations. Various devices have been used. For instance, the electrode may be constructed as shown in the diagram (Fig. 44). A short glass tube of a bore of about 4 mms. is well cleaned — one end, which is to come into contact with the nerve — is filled, as ■shown, by a plug of kaolin made into a stiff putty with physiological saline solution of NaCl (0.7 per cent.). The kaolin should have a neutral reaction and unless good kaolin is obtainable it is better to use a plug made of clean filter paper macerated in physiological saline and packed tightly into the end of the tube. Above this plug the tube is filled in for a part of its length with a saturated solution of zinc sulphate into which is immersed a bar of amal- gamated zinc with a copper wire soldered to its end. With a pair of such electrodes the conduction of the current through the nerve or muscle to the hi i't;tllic part of the circuit may be represented as follows: Zn + Zn + Zn + Na + Na + Na + Zn + Zn SO., SO, CI CI CI so4 so4 Zn The liquid part of the circuit comes into contact with the metallic part at the junction of Zn and ZnS04. At the cathode it may be supposed that the Zu cation instead of acting upon the water and liberating hydrogen, deposits itself upon the zinc electrode; at the anode the sulphion (S< )4) attacks the zinc instead of the water, forming ZnS( ).,. ]n this way polarization is prevented, and by the construction of the electrode! the living tissue is brought into contact only with the plug of kaolin moistened with physio- logical saline, Sucfa electrodes are indispensable in studying the electrical phe- nomena of living tissues, and also in all investigations bearing upon I, he polar effect during the passage of an electrical current from a battery. Ordinarily^ however, when it is only desired to stimulate a nerve or muscle, metal (plat- inum) electrodes are employed. ELECTRICAL PHENOMENA. 103 The Action Current or Negative Variation. — Du Bois-Rey- mond proved that when the excised muscle or nerve is stimulated its demarcation current suffers a diminution or negative variation. If, for instance, the excised nerve gives a demarcation current suf- ficient to cause a deflection in the galvanometer of 50 mms., then if the nerve is stimulated by a series of induction shocks the galva- nometer will show a lessened deflection, say, one of 40 mms. The negative variation in this case is equal to 10 mms., on the scale of the galvanometer used. It has been shown that this negative varia- tion is due to a current in the opposite direction whose strength, in the example given, relative to that of the demarcation current is as 10 to 50. Frequently the phenomenon of the negative varia- tion is known also as the action current. The explanation given for this action current is that the nerve or muscle when excited takes on an electrical condition which is negative as regards any unexcited or less excited portion of the nerve. The effect upon the demarcation current is illustrated in the accompanying diagram. The demarcation current in a nerve is led off to a galvanometer by electrodes placed at 6 and c. When the nerve is stimulated at a the excitation set up passes along the nerve, and wherever it may be that portion of the nerve is thrown into an electronegative condi- tion. When this condition reaches a point at which it can influence the galvanometer — that is, when it reaches b, it will diminish the difference in potential that exists between b and c, and therefore reduce the current flowing from b to c. _f_ Bernstein* has shown that this neg- ative condition moves in the form of a wave. That is, at any point the nega- tivity grOWS to a Fig 45.— Schema to indicate the method of detecting maximum and then ^e action current in a stimulated excised nerve: b and c, the leading on electrodes, one on the longitudinal, one on diminishes. More- the cut surface; the demarcation current passes through . the galvanometer, g, in the direction of the arrows; a, stimu- OVer, it travels at a lating electrodes from induction coil; the stimulus causes a , „ . , . , negative condition, — which passes along the nerve ; when definite VelOClty this reaches bit causes a partial reversal of the demarca- t_ • 1 • • 1 tion current, giving the negative variation or action cur- which is easily rent. measured. Accord- ing to his experiments, the velocity of this wave in the frog's motor nerve is from 25 to 28 meters per second, and the length of the wave is about 18 mms. Hermann, on the contrary, be- lieves that, in the excised nerve at least, the length of the wave may be greater, reaching perhaps 140 mms. * Bernstein, " Untersuchungen uber den Erregungsvorgang im Nerven und Muskelsysteme," Heidelberg 1871. 104 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. These figures will vary naturally for the nerves of different ani- mals or for different nerves in the same animal, for it must always be remembered that nerve fibers, whose functions in general are so similar, differ much in obvious microscopical structure and probably more widely in their chemical composition. Using an analogy that is familiar, we may say that when a stimulus acts upon a living nerve a wave of electronegativity spreads from the stimulated spot and travels in wave form witl a definite velocity, just as water waves radiate from the spot at which a stone is thrown into a quiet pool. A similar phenomenon occurs in muscle fibers when stimu- lated, but the negative condition travels over the muscle fiber at a slower speed, 3 to 4 meters per second in frog's muscle, and with a wave length, according to Bernstein, of only 10 nuns. This wave of negativity in the muscle begins during the latent period and, therefore, precedes the actual shortening at any point, as shown in Fig. 48. This phenomenon of a negative electrical condition traveling over the nerve or muscle and giving us an active current when led off through a galvanometer is of the greatest physiological impor- tance, particularly in the study of nerves. It has been shown that in the nerve this wave of negativity marks the progress of the wave of excitation, and, since we can study its progress by means of the galvanometer or capillary electrometer, we can thus study the excitability and conductivity in nerves when removed from con- nection with their end-organs. That the negative wave, or the action current that it gives rise to, is an invariable sign of the passage of an excitation or nerve impulse is shown by the facts that. it is absent in the dead nerve, and that in the living nerve it is produced by mechanical,* chemical, f and reflexj stimulations, as well as by the more usual method of electrical stimulation. Herzen lias claimed that under certain conditions of local narcosis the nerve fibers when stimulated may give an action current, hut no muscle con- traction,— a fact which if true would seem to show that the excitation wave or nerve impulse and the wave of negative potential are not associated invariably. This result, however, has been denied by other competent observers (Wedenski, Boruttau). Monophasic and Diphasic Action Currents. — According to the conception of the action current given above, it is evident that it should be obtained upon stimulation when a living normal nerve Is connected at any two points of its course with a galvanometer or capillary electrometer. The detection of the current under such * Steinach, " Pfliiger's Archiv," 55. 487, 1894. tGriitzner, "Pfliiger's Archiv," 25, 2.r,r,, L881. X Boruttau, " Pfliiger's Archiv," si and 90, 1001-1902. ELECTRICAL PHENOMENA. 105 conditions offers more difficulties, because it is diaphasic, as will be seen from the accompanying diagram (Fig. 46). The figure represents a normal nerve led off to the galvanometer from two points, b and c, of its longitudinal surface. As these points in the uninjured nerve have the same potential, no current is shown by the galvanometer. If the nerve is stimulated at a by a single stimulus, a negative condition or charge passes along the nerve. When it reaches the point b, there will be a momentary current through the galvanometer from c to b; as the charge passes on to c, this point in turn will become negative to b, and there will be a momentary current through the galvanometer in the other direction. The diphasic current that occurs under these con- ditions cannot be detected by the ordinary galvanometer, even when a series of stimuli is sent into the nerve at a, since the movable system in this instrument has too much inertia to respond to such quick changes in opposite directions. With the more mobile string-galvanometer or capillary electrometer the diphasic currents have been demonstrated successfully. In laboratory investigations one of the leading off electrodes, c, is usually placed on the cut end of the nerve. Under this con- dition the action current becomes monophasic and shows itself as a negative variation of the demarcation current. This difference is due to the fact that a negative condition upon excitation depends upon a living condition of the nerve, and it cannot, therefore, affect the nerve at the electrode c if this latter is placed upon the cut end where the nerve is dead or dying. It will affect only the electrode b, and give only the monophasic current, which can now be shown by the usual galvanometer, provided a series of stimuli is thrown in at a. Fig. 46. — Schema to show the arrangement for obtaining a diphasic action current. The arrangement differs from that in Fig. 42 only in that both leading off electrodes, b and c, are placed on the longitudinal surface. No demarcation current is indicated. When the nerve is stimulated at a the negative charge reaches b first, causing a current through the galvanometer from c to b. Subsequently it reaches c and causes a second current in the opposite direction from b to c. The Positive Variation. — It happens not infrequently that when one electrode is placed upon the cut end, the nerve upon stimulation with a series of induction shocks gives a positive instead of a negative variation of the demarcation current. This result is usually explained as being due to a pre- dominance of the anelectrotonic currents (see below), but Wedenski has con- tended recently that it is due to a peculiar condition of excitation in the nerve 106 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE \XD NERVE. at the cut end, a condition to which he gives the name of parabiosis. When this phenomenon occurs it can usually be removed by making a fresh section at the end of the nerve. Detection of the Action Currents by the Rheoscopic Frog Preparation or by the Telephone. — The motor nerve of a nerve- muscle preparation from a frog is so extremely irritable to electrical currents that it may be used instead of a galvanometer to detect the action currents in a stimulated muscle. A nerve-muscle prep- aration used for this purpose is known as a rheoscopic preparation. The way in which it is used is indicated in the accompanying diagram, b represents the rheoscopic preparation, its nerve being laid upon the muscle whose currents are being investigated, a, so as to touch the cut end (x) and the longitudinal surface (g) . When a is stimulated, either directly or through its nerve, as represented in the diagram; the negative charges that pass along the muscle fibers of a with each stimulus cause action currents that will be led off through the nerve of b from x to g. If the nerve is in a sensitive con- dition it will be stimulated by the action currents and thus a series of excitations will be sent into b corresponding exactly in rate with the artificial stimuli given to the nerve of a. The rheoscopic preparation may be used very beautifully to demonstrate the action current in the contracting heart muscle. If the nerve of b is laid upon the exposed beating heart of an animal, the muscle of b will give a single twitch for each beat of the ventricle. An- other interesting method of detecting the action currents, particu- larly in nerves, is by means of the telephone. Wedenski has made especial use of this method, the telephone being connected with lie. 17. Si-licma to .show (lie arrangement of a rheoscopic muscle-nerve preparation: b, The rheoscopic muscle-nerve preparation, the nerve being; arranged to touch the out but- jape and the longitudinal mrface of the musole, a, whose action ourrenta are to !»■ detected. When I be nerve Of a is stimulal'd cadi cun t.r:ifl inn of tliis muscle in followed l>y u contrac- tion of I), .since each conl rail ion of a is accompanied by an action current wliich passes throuicli the nerve of b ami stimulates it. the nerve in place of the galvanometer. • The method has obvious advantages in the fact that it may be used with a nerve to which the muscle is also attached, SO that the excitation processes in the nerve and their effect upon the muscle may be studied simul- taneously. ELECTRICAL PHENOMENA. 107 Relation of the Action Current to the Contraction Wave in Muscle and to the Excitation Wave (Nerve Impulse) in Nerve. — The action current or, to be more accurate, the moving negative charge which gives rise to an action current when two points of the muscle are led off to a galvanometer, has been shown by Bernstein to precede the wave of contraction in muscle; that is, in a stimulated muscle fiber the electrical change at any point precedes the mechanical process of. shortening. When studied by means of the string-galvanometer it would seem, according to the curve reproduced in Fig. 48, that in a simple Fig. 48. — Photograph of the electrical variation in the frog's gastrocnemius muscle ■during a simple contraction, as given by the string-galvanometer : a. The electrical curve, showing two waves ; b (retouched to make it distinct), occurring during the latent period. This is followed by a smaller but longer wave, which begins at the moment of shortening of the muscle ; c, the break in this line indicates the moment of stimulation ; d, the curve of contraction of the muscle ; k, vibrations of a tuning-fork at the rate of 100 per second. — (Judin.) muscular contraction two electrical waves pass over the muscle: first, a quick extensive change in potential which occurs during the latent period and marks probably the passage of the wave of excitation; second, a slower wave accompanying the proc- ess of shortening. Paying attention only to the first of these waves, we may suppose that the electrical change is an indication of the excitation Or possibly constitutes the excitation that sets up the chemical change of contraction, or else that the change in electrical potential is caused by the chemical change of contraction and precedes the mechanical result of shortening, since the latter process will have a certain latent period. It has been shown, indeed, by Demoor that a completely fatigued muscle may still 10S THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. conduct an excitation (muscle impulse), although unable to con- tract, and the same fact has been demonstrated by Engelmann for the heart muscle. In the nerve the action current, or the negative change causing it, has been considered as simultaneous with or possibly identical with the nerve impulse. The velocity of the two is identical; the action current is given whenever the nerve is stimulated, and, so far as experiments have gone, the nerve cannot enter into activity without showing an action current. — that is. without showing a moving electrical charge. Whether this electrical charge constitutes the nerve impulse or is simply an accompanying phenomenon will be discussed briefly in the paragraph upon the nature of the nerve impulse in the following chapter. The Electrotonic Currents. — In speaking of the effect of passing a galvanic current through a nerve attention was called to the fact that the condition of the nerve is altered at each pole. At the anode there is a con- dition of decreased irritability and con- ductivity known as anelectrotonus ; a t the cathode, in the beginning, at least, a condition of in- creased irritability known as catelec- trotonus. In addi- tion to these changes in the physiological properties of the nerve there is a change also in its electrical condition at each pole, of such a character that if the nerve is led off from two points on the anode side a current will be indicated. The current can be obtained at a considerable distance from the anode, and is known as the anelectrotonic current, while the electrical condition in the nerve fchat makes it possible is designated as anelectrotonus. A similar current can be led off from the nerve on the cathode side for a considerable distance beyond the cathode; this is known as the catelectrotonic current, and the electrical condition leading to its production as catelectrotonus. Within the nerve these electrotonic currents have the same direction as the battery or polarizing current, as is shown in the diagram (Fig. 49), The terms anelectrotonus and catelectrotonus are used, therefore, in physiology to designate both the physiological and the elec- Fig. 49. — Schema to show the direction of the elec- trotonic currents in an excised nerve: P, The battery for the polarizing current sent into the nerve at +, the an- ode, and emerging at — , the cathode; o\ galvanometer arranged with leading off electrodes to detect the anelec- trotonic current, the direction of which is indicated by the arrows (in the nerve it is the same as that of the po- .larizing current); g, galvanometer similarly arranged to de- tect the catelectrotonic current. The anelectrotonic and catelectrotonic currents continue as long as the polarizing current is maintained. ELECTRICAL PHENOMENA. 109 trical changes around the poles when a battery current is led into a nerve. Whether the physiological and the electrical changes have a causal connection or are two independent phenomena is at present undecided. Bethe* has recently shown that during the passage of the polarizing cur- rent the neurofibrils in the axis cylinder lose at the anode their power of stain- ing with certain basic dyes (c. g. , methylene blue) , while at the cathode the affinity for these dyes is increased. He assumes, that in the neurofibrils there is an acid substance — fibril acid — and that at the anode the combination with this body and the neurofibrils is loosened; hence the loss of staining power. At the cathode the reverse change takes place. He assumes further- Fig. 50. — To show the action of the core-model: p, The polarizing current; g' and g, the galvanometers with leading off electrodes to detect the anelectrotonic and catelec* trotonic currents, respectively. more, that when the affinity between neurofibril and fibril acid is increased at the cathode an electronegative ion is liberated (anion), while at the anode at the time that the combination between fibril and fibril acid is dis- sociated an electropositive ion (cation) is liberated. In this way he constructs an hypothesis of a complex of neurofibril, fibril acid, and electrolyte which is capable of accounting for the electrotonus, both as regards the electrical and the physiological phenomena, and which refers both phenomena to a single reaction in the nerve. Another explanation of the electrotonic currents which has been much discussed is that first developed by Hermann. f This author constructed a model consisting of a conductor surrounded by a less conductive liquid sheath, and showed that such a model is capable of giving the electrotonic currents. This model may be made as represented in the accompanying diagram, of a glass tube A-B, through the middle of which is stretched a platinum wire, P, the rest of the tube being filled with a saturated solution of zinc sulphate. The glass tube is provided with vertical branches by means of which a polarizing current, p, can be sent into the solution of zinc sulphate and the electrotonic currents be led off to galvanometers, g', g, on each side. Under these conditions a current similar to the anelectrotonic current can be detected on the side of the anode {g') and one equivalent to the catelectrotonic current on the side of the cathode (g). The explanation given to these currents is that as the threads of current pass into the platinum core there is a polarization at the surface between the core and the zinc sul- phate solution which extends to a considerable distance on each side of the electrodes and causes diffusion currents from sheath to core. It is these threads of current that may be led off as electrotonic currents. Hermann suggested that hi the nerve we have a structure essentially similar to that of the core model. He thought that the axis cylinder might be considered as representing the core and the myelin the less conductive sheath corre- sponding to the zinc sulphate solution. Others (Boruttau) have suggested that * Bethe, " Allgemeine Anatomie u. Physiol, des Nervensystems," Leipzig, 1903. f Hermann, " Handbuch der Physiologie," vol. ii, p. 174. 110 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. the neurofibrils in the axis cylinder may represent the core or cores and the sur- rounding neuroplasm the sheath, thus providing for the possibility of electro- tonic currents in non-medullated fibers. As a matter of fact, the non-medul- lated fibers in mammals give very slight electrotonic currents compared with the medullated fibers.* According to the "core-model" explanation, the electrotonic currents represent a purely physical phenomenon, which is dependent, however, upon a certain structure of the nerve. That is, a completely dead nerve will not show these currents, although an anesthetized nerve, in the mammal (Waller) at least, continues to show them, and, according to Sosnowsky, excised rab- bits' nerves kept in a moist atmosphere may show them for several days. While the core-model hypothesis has led to much investigation in physiology and has been made the basis for a purely physical explanation of the nerve impulse, it is still very uncertain whether it furnishes any positive informa- tion concerning the processes that actually take place in the living nerve wheD submitted to the action of electrical currents or other artificial stimuli. *Alcock, "Proceedings Royal Society," 1904, 73, p. 166. CHAPTER V. THE NATURE OF THE NERVE IMPULSE AND THE NUTRITIVE RELATIONS OF NERVE FIBER AND NERVE CELL. The question of the nature of the nerve impulse has always aroused the deepest interest among physiologists. It has consti- tuted, indeed, a central question around which have revolved vari- ous hypotheses concerning the nature of living matter. The impor- tance of the nerves as conductors of motion and sensation was apparent to the old physiologists, and the nature of the conduction or the thing conducted was the subject of many hypotheses and many different names. For many years the prevalent view was that the nerves are essentially tubes through which flows an ex- ceedingly fine matter, of the nature of air or gas, known as the animal spirits. Others conceived this fluid to be of a grosser struc- ture like water and described it as the nerve juice. With Galvani's discovery of electricity the nerve principle, as it was called, became identified with electricity, and, indeed, this view, as will be ex- plained, occurs in modified form to-day. Du Bois-Reymond, after discovering the demarcation current and action current in muscle and nerve, formulated an hypothesis according to which the nerve fibers contain a series of electromotive particles, and by this hypothesis and the facts upon which it was based he thought that he had established that " hundred-year-old dream" of phys- icists and physiologists of the identity of the nerve principle and electricity. His theory to-day has fallen into disrepute, but the facts upon which it was based remain, as before, of the deepest importance. In the middle of the nineteenth century those who were not convinced of the identity of the nerve principle with electricity believed, nevertheless, that the process of conduction in the nerve is a phenomenon of an order comparable to the trans- mission of light or electricity, with a velocity so great as to defy measurement. But in this same period a simple but complete experiment by Helmholtz demonstrated that its velocity is, as compared with light or with electrical conduction through the air or through metals, exceedingly slow, — 27 meters per second. Modern views have taken divergent directions; the movement or excitation that is conducted along the fiber has been named 111 112 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. the nerve principle, the nerve energy, the nerve force, the nerve impulse. As the latter term is less specific regarding the nature of the movement, and emphasizes the fact of the conduction of an isolated disturbance or pulse, it seems preferable to employ it until a more satisfactory solution of its nature has been reached. The Velocity of the Nerve Impulse. — The determination of the velocity of the nerve impulse was first made by Helmholtz* upon the motor nerves of frogs. His experiment consisted in stimulating the sciatic nerve, first, near its ending in the muscle Fig. 51. — Record to show the method of estimating the velocity of the nerve impulse in a motor nerve. The experiment was made upon a nerve-muscle preparation from the frog, the contractions bring recorded upon the rapidly moving plate of a pendulum myo- graph. Two contractions were obtained, the first (a) when the nerve was stimulated near the mu-.rle the -ennui ill) when the nerve was stimulated as far as possible from the muscle. The latent period of the second Contraction was longer, as shown by the distance between the curses measured on the line x. The value of this distance in time Ls obtained rence to the record of a tuning fork vibrating 100 times per second, which is given on the lower line. In the experiment the length of a tuning fork wave (0.01 sec.) was L'l nuns., the .li-lan«-e l.ct^-een the two muscular contractions was 3.35 mms., and the dis- tance between the point i stimulated upon the nerve was 19 mms. Hence the velocity of the nerveimpul e in thi experiment was 49 divided by (^'Wo ;X ,,',,,) oiiiiiTHimms. Cio.Tltim.) per second. and, second, near its origin from the cord, and measuring the time fli.it elapsed in each case between the moment of stimulation and the moment of the muscular response. It was found that when the nerve was stimulated at its far end this time interval was longer, and since .'ill oilier conditions remained the same this dif- ference in time could only be due to the interval required for the nerve impulse to travel the longer stretch of nerve. In the accom- * Helmholtz, "Miiller'a Archiv f. \...-.t. u. Physiol.," L852, p. 199. NATURE OF THE NERVE IMPULSE. 113 panying figure the record of a laboratory experiment of this kind is reproduced. Knowing the difference in time and also the length of nerve between the points stimulated, the data are at hand to calculate the velocity of the impulse. The velocity varies with the temperature. According to Helmholtz, this variation lies between 24.6 and 38.4 m. per second for a range of temperature between 11° and 21° C. For average room temperatures we may say that in the motor nerves of the frog the impulse travels with a velocity of 28 to 30 meters per second. Similar experiments have been made upon man and other mammals. Helmholtz stimulated the median nerve in man at two different points and recorded the resulting contractions of the muscles of the thumb. By this means he obtained an average velocity of 34 m. per second, but others, making use of the same method, have reported varying results. Quite recently Piper* has applied the string- galvanometer to the investigation of this point. Using the unipolar method, he stimulated the median nerve with induction shocks, the active electrode being applied at the elbow and at the axilla at a distance apart of from 160 to 170 mm. The muscular response was recorded not by registering the con- traction, but by means of its action current When the stimulus was applied at the elbow the interval between the stimulation and the electrical response averaged 0.00442 second; at the axilla the interval was 0.00578 second. The difference, namely, 0.00136 second, gave the time necessary for the impulse to travel over 160 to 170 mm. of nerve, and indicated a velocity of 117 to 125 m. per second. It is interesting to recall that only six years before Helmholtz's first pub- lication Johannes Muller had stated that we should never find a means of determining the velocity of the nerve impulse, since it would be impossible to compare points at great distances apart, as in the case of the movement of light. " The time," said he, " required for the transmission of a sensation from the periphery to the brain and the return reflex movements of the mus- cles is infinitely small and unmeasurable." The mode of reasoning by which Helmholtz was led to doubt the validity of this assertion is interesting. He says (" Midler's Archiv," 1852,330): "As long as physiologists thought it necessary to refer nerve actions to the movement of an imponderable or psychical principle, it must have appeared incredible that the velocity of this movement could be measured within the short distances of the animal body. At present we know from the researches of du Bois-Rej-mond upon the electro- motive properties of nerves that those activities by means of which the con- duction of an excitation is accomplished are in reality actually conditioned by, or at least closely connected with an altered arrangement of their materia) particles. Therefore conduction in nerves must belong to the series of self- propagating reactions of ponderable bodies, such, for example, as the con- duction of sound in the air or elastic structures, or the combustions in a tube filled with an explosive mixture." One of the first fruits, therefore, of the scientific investigation of the electrical properties of the nerve fiber was the discovery of the important fact of the velocity of the nerve impulse. * Piper, "Archiv f. d. ges. Physiologic," 1908, 124, 591. 114 THK PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. Numerous efforts have been made to determine the velocity of the nerve impulse in medullated sensory fibers. The results have not been entirely satisfactory. The end-organ in this case is the cortex of the cerebrum, and its reaction consists in arousing a sensation, or a reflex action. Neither end-reaction can be meas- ured directly. Attempts have been made to determine it indi- rectly by noting the time of a voluntary muscle response for sensory stimuli applied to the skin at different distances from the spinal axis. In such cases the sensory impulse travels to the cord, thence to the brain, and the return motor impulse travels from brain to cord and then by the motor nerves to the muscle used for the re- sponse. The results of this method have been discordant, owing probably to the fact that the central paths from two different points on the skin are not identical. It is usually assumed — without, however, very convincing proof — that the velocity of the impulse in the medullated afferent nerve fibers is the same as in the efferent fibers. A large number of observations are on record which show that the velocity varies greatly in the nerves of different animals. In the mammal, according to Chauveau, the velocity for the non-medullated fibers is only 8 meters per second; in the lobster it is 6 meters per second; in the octopus, 2 meters; in the olfactory (sensory) nerve of the pike, -g- meter, and in the anodon, only tttt meter per second. Relation of the Nerve Impulse to the Wave of Negativity. — A fact of great significance is that the velocity of the impulse in the motor nerves of the frog corresponds exactly to the velocity of the wave of negativity as measured by Bernstein. Evidently the two phenomena are coincident in their progress along the fiber, and physiologists generally have accepted the existence of an action cur- rent as a proof of the passage of a nerve impulse. This belief is strengthened by the fact that, as stated above, the negative wave ac- companies the nerve impulse not only when the nerve is stimulated by electrical currents, but also after mechanical, chemical, or reflex si jiiiiil.il ion. The question has been raised as to whether this elec- trical phenomenon accompanies the normal nerve impulse, — that is, the nerve impulse that originates in the nerve centers, in the case of motor nerves, or in the peripheral sense organs in the case of sen- sory nerves. In regard to the latter relation we have positive evi- dence t hat when light falls upon the living retina an electrical distur- bance i produced by the visible rays of the spectrum,* and there is ever, reason to believe that the passage of visual impulses along the optic nerve is accompanied by an electrical change. With regard to normal motor impulses, the evidence is also positive that motor discharges from t he central nervous system are accompanied ♦ Consult Gotch, "Journal of Physiology," 31, I, L904. NATURE OF THE NERVE IMPULSE. 115 by a wave of electrical potential. This fact may be shown by stimulating the motor areas in the cerebral cortex and testing the efferent nerves, such as the sciatic, for an action current; or by stimulating a posterior root on one side in the lumbar region and testing the sciatic nerve on the other side with a galvanometer.* Moreover, all influences that alter the velocity or strength of the nerve impulse affect the intensity of the action current in the same manner. It is believed generally, therefore, that the electrical charge is an invariable accompaniment of the excitatory wave, and the demonstration of an action current in a nerve is tantamount to a proof of the passage of a nerve impulse. Direction of Conduction in the Nerve. — The fact that under normal conditions the motor fibers conduct impulses only in one direction — i. e.f toward the periphery — and the sensory fibers in the opposite direction — that is, toward the nerve center — suggests, of course, the question as to whether the direction of conduction is conditioned by a fundamental difference in structure in the two kinds of fibers. No such difference in structure has been revealed by the microscope, although in two respects at least it will be re- membered that the sensory nerve fibers react differently from the motor fibers — namely, in the fact that they are readily stimulated by high temperatures and that during the passage of a galvanic current of constant strength they are stimulated continuously in- stead of only at the opening or closing of the current. These latter differences, however, may rest simply upon a difference in irrita- bility and have no bearing upon the question in hand. It is the accepted belief in physiology that any nerve fiber may conduct an impulse in both directions, and does so conduct its impulses when the fiber is stimulated in the middle of its course. An entirely satisfactory proof for this belief is difficult to furnish unless the conclusion in the preceding para- graph is admitted, — the conclusion, namely, that the electrical change is a necessary and in- variable accompani- ment of the nerve Fig. 52.— Schema to show the arrangement for proving . T . the propagation of the negative charge in both directions: impulse. It IS not a. The stimulating electrodes; g and g', galvanometers t rr- ill i l with leading off electrodes arranged to show the negative difficult to shOW by variation on each side. means of a galva- nometer that when a nerve trunk is stimulated the negative charge spreads in both directions from the point stimulated and * Gotch and Horsley, "Phil. Trans., Royal Soc," London, 1891, vol. 182 (B), and Boruttau, " Pfluger's Archiv," 1901. 116 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. gives an active current on either side, as indicated in the accom- panying diagram. This fact holds true for motor or for sensory fibers. The older physiologists attempted to settle this question in a more direct way, but by methods which later experiments have proved to be insufficient. They attempted, for instance, to unite a motor and sensory trunk directly, to cut the hypoglossal (motor) and the lingual (sensory) and suture, say, the central stump of the lingual to the peripheral stump of the hypoglossal. If stimu- lation of this latter trunk, after union had been established, gave signs of sensation it was considered as proof that the efferent hypo- glossal fibers were now conducting afferently. We now know that in such a case the old hypoglossal fibers degenerate completely, and the new ones that are eventually formed in their place are out- growths from the lingual stump, or at least are not the old efferent fibers, and hence experiments of this kind are not so conclusive as they seemed to be at the time when it was supposed that severed nerve fibers can unite immediately, by first intention, without previous degeneration. A similar objection applies to Paul Bert's often quoted experiment. Bert implanted the tip of a rat's tail into the skin of its back. After union had taken place the tail was severed at the base, and the stump now attached to the back was tested from time to time as to its sensibility. Sensation returned slowly. At first it was indefinite, but by the end of a year was apparently normal. Modification of the Nerve Impulse by Various Influences — Narcosis — Temperature. — The strength of the impulse and its velocity may be modified in various ways: by the action of temperature, narcotics, pressure, etc. Variations of tempera- ture, as stated before, change the velocity of propagation of the impulse, the velocity increasing with a rise of temperature up to a certain point. So also the irritability as well as the con- ductivity of the nerve fiber is influenced markedly by tem- perature. If a small area: of a nerve trunk be cooled or heated, the nerve impulse as it, passes through this area, may be increased or decreased in strength or may be blocked entirely. Different fiher.s show somewhat different reactions in this respect,; but, speaking generally, the limits of conductivity in relation to temperature Lie between 0° C. and 50° ('. Cooling a nerve to n C. will in most cases suspend the conductivity, but this function returns promptly upon warming.* By this means we can Mock the nerve impulses in a nerve trunk for any desired lengl h of i ime. The exact relai ionship between the temperature of the nerve and the velocity of the impulse has been studied carefully with the object of determining the temperature coejji- * Howell, Budgett, and Leonard, "Journal of Physiology," 16, 298, L894. NATURE OF THE NERVE IMPULSE. 117 dent. It has been shown by van't Hoff that the velocity of chemical reactions is increased twofold or more for each rise of 10 degrees in temperature, that is, the temperature coefficient for chemical reactions lies between 2 and 3. On the other hand, with most physical processes the temperature coefficient for the same range of temperature lies around 1 or between 1 and 2. Snyder* finds, on comparing the velocities of the impulse at different temperatures, that they follow van't Hoff's law for chemical reactions, that is, the velocity is approximately doubled by a rise of 10° C. in temperature within physiological limits, , • i + velocity at fn + 10 9 T, • or, expressed in more general terms, — -. — r- -j =2. 1 nis effect of temperature on the velocity of the impulse is shown graphically in Fig. 53. Anesthetics and narcotics, J such as ether, Fig. 53. — Figure to show the effect of temperature on the velocity of the nerve impulse. At each temperature two contractions of the gastrocnemius were recorded, one when the nerve was stimulated close to the muscle, one when it was stimulated further away (44 mm.). The horizontal distance between the curves as they rise can be expressed in time by refer- ence to the tuning-fork vibrations (200 per second) given below. For intervals of 10° C. it will be seen that the velocity, as indicated by the reciprocals of the distances between the pairs of curves, indicates a coefficient of two. — (Snyder.) chloroform, cocain, chloral, phenol, alcohol, etc., may be applied locally to a nerve trunk, and if the application is made with care the conductivity and irritability may be lessened or suspended entirely at that point, to be restored again when the narcotic is removed. It is an interesting fact that the conductivity of the nerve may be suspended also by deprivation of oxygen, J — that is, by local suffocation or asphyxia. A nerve fiber sur- rounded by an oxygen-free atmosphere will slowly lose its conductivity, and this property will be restored promptly upon the admission of oxygen. Compression of a nerve will also suspend its conductivity without permanently injuring the fibers, provided the pressure is properly graduated. Lastly, * Snyder, "American Journal of Physiology, " 22, 179, 1908. fFrohlich, "Zeitschrift f. allgemeine Physiol.," 3, 75, 1903. t Baeyers, ibid., 2, 169, 1903. 118 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. as was explained in a preceding chapter, the conductivity of the nerve may be increased or decreased or suspended entirety by the action of a galvanic (polarizing) current. This method of sus- pending conductivity temporarily has been frequently employed for experimental purposes, the arrangement being as represented in Fig. 54. "When the conductivity of the nerve is interrupted by any of the methods described above, certain peculiar reactions may be obtained in the inter- mediate stages before conduction is entirely abolished. The most interest- ing of the stages is the paradoxical condition. In this stage a weak stimulus applied at a will cause a contraction of the muscle, while a stronger stimulus will prove ineffective. Wedenski,* who has studied these reactions with great care, believes that the nerve in the narcotized area is thrown into a peculiar condition of continued excitation to which he gives the name of parabiosis. The condition is supposed to be characterized physiologically by a loss of lability of the living material. It seems possible, however, that the reactions which are taken as characteristic of the parabiotic condition may be explained upon the assumption that the narcotics and other reagents mentioned so alter the nerve as to make it more susceptible to fatigue (see following paragraph). The Question of Fatigue of Nerve Fibers. — An important question in connection with the nature of the nerve impulse has been that of the suscep- tibility of the nerve fibers to fatigue. The obvious fatigue of muscles and of nerve centers has been referred to the accumula- tion of the products of metabolism of their tis- sues or to the actual consumption of the en- ergy-yielding material in them. Functional activ- ity in these tissues im- plies the breaking down of complex organic material (catabolism) and the setting free of the so-called chemical energy. The potential, chemical or internal energy of the compound is liberated as kinetic energy of heat, etc. It has been accepted, therefore, that, if the nerve fiber could be demonstrated to show fatigue as a result of functional activity, this fact would be probable proof that the conduction of the im- pulse is associated with a chemical change of a catabolic nature in the substance of the fiber. Experimental work, however, has shown that under normal conditions the nerve fiber shows no fatigue. The experiments made upon this point have been nu- * Wedenski, "Pfluger'a Arehiv," 100, 1, 1904. Fig. 54. — Schema to show the method of block- ing the nerve impulse by means of a polarizing cur- rent: a. The stimulating electrodes; 6, the battery, the current of which is led into the nerve. The de- pressed irritability at both anode, +, and cathode, — , prevents the nerve impulse started at a from reaching the muscle. NATURE OF THE NERVE IMPULSE. 119 merous and varied. The general idea underlying all of them has been to stimulate the nerve continuously, but to interpose a block somewhere along the course of the nerve so that the impulses should not reach the end-organ. This precaution is necessary because the end-organ- — muscle, gland, etc. — is subject to fatigue, and must therefore be protected from constant activity. From time to time or at the end of a long period of stimulation the block is removed and it is noted whether or not the end-organ — for in- stance, the muscle — gives signs of a stimulation. The removable block has been obtained by the action of a polarizing current, by cold, by narcotics, by curare, etc. Using curare, for instance, Bowditch* found that the sciatic nerve might be stimulated continu- ously by induction shocks for several (four to five) hours without complete fatigue, since as the curare effect wore off the muscle whose contractions were being recorded (M. tibialis ant.) began to respond, at first with single and finally with tetanic contractions. The curare in this case may be supposed to have blocked the nerve impulse at the motor end-plate and thus protected the muscle from responding until the lapse of several hours, although the nerve was under stimulation during this entire time. This experiment has since been repeated by Durig,f who has made use of the fact that the effects of curare can be removed within a few minutes by the salicylate of physostigmin. Durig stimulated the nerve for as much as ten hours and then upon removing the curare block found from the contraction of the muscle that the nerve was still conducting. EdesJ and others have shown that the same result is obtained when the nerve is tested by a capillary electrometer instead of by the response of an end-organ. Under such conditions the nerve exhibits an undiminished action cur- rent, although constantly stimulated by tetanizing shocks from an induction apparatus. Brodie and Halliburton! have found that the non-medullated fibers in the splenic nerve can also be stimulated for many hours without losing their power of conduction, — that is, without showing fatigue. Many other observers have obtained similar results, which have confirmed physiologists in the belief that the nerve fibers may conduct impulses indefinitely, or, in other words, that their normal functional activity may be carried on continuously without fatigue. If this belief is entirely correct it would place the nerve fibers in a class by themselves, since all other tissues that have been studied show evidence of fatigue when kept in continuous functional activity. Moreover, if this belief is entirely correct it would imply that the conduction of an impulse * Bowditch, " Journal of Physiology," 6, 133, 1885. t Durig, " Centralblatt f. Physiol.," 15, 751, 1902. t Edes", "Journal of Physiology," 13, 431, 1892. § Brodie and Halliburton, "Journal of Physiology," 28, 181, 1902. 12C THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND XERYE. in the nerve fiber is not associated with a consumption of material, a metabolism, and in this respect also the functional activity of the nerve would be placed in contrast with that of other organs. It must be remembered, however, that, although the above experiments demonstrate the practical "unfatigueableness" of nerve fibers under ordinary conditions of stimulation, there are some reasons to make us hesitate in supposing that in these structures functional activity is always without a depressing effect upon irritability. In the first place it has been shown that the nerve exhibits the phenomenon of a " refractory period." That is to say, for a certain brief interval after stimulation it is in a non-irritable condition. If two stimuli be applied to a nerve with a very brief interval between (0.006 sec. or less, according to the temperature), the second stimulus is ineffective so far as can be determined by the response of an attached muscle or by means of a capillary electrometer.* It may very well be that in this case the lack of response to the second stimulus is due to a short-lasting fatigue from the first stimulus. This point of view is strengthened by the fact that, when the irritability of the nerve is greatly depressed by narcotics, f this critical interval is much lengthened; two stimuli with a rate of more than 10 per second may give an effect only for the first stimulus. Garten has shown that one nerve, the olfactory of the pike, when stimulated by induction shocks, with an interval between the stimuli of as much as 0.27 sec., gives evidence of fatigue, since its action current as measured by the capillary electrometer diminishes in extent quite rapidly, and recovers after a short rest.| So also it has boon found that while a nerve deprived of oxygen, by keeping it in an atmosphere of nitrogen, loses its irritability after a certain time, this event occurs much more rapidly if the nerve is stimulated constantly. § This fact would suggest, therefore;, that the ability of the nerve under normal eircumstances to escape the results of fatigue may be due possibly to the fact that the supply of oxygen is sufficiently abundant to oxidize the fatigue substances formed during activity. Does the Nerve Fiber Show Any Evidence of Metabolism during Functional Activity? The functional pari of a nerve filicr in conduction IS the axis cylinder, and, indeed, probably the neurofibrils in the axis cylinder. The mass of this material even in a Large nerve t runk is small, and its chemisl ry is bul little known. '. »tch and Burch, "Journal of Physiology, " 24, 410, L899. i I rdhlich, "Zeitschrift I', allgemeine Physiol.,"3, 468, L904. | Quoted from Bierdermann, " Ergebnisse der Phyeiologie, " vol. ii, part ii, p. 129. § Tliorncr, " Zeitechrifl f. flllg. Physiologic," S, 530, 1908. NATURE OF THE NERVE IMPULSE. 121 The efforts that have been made to prove a metabolism in the nerve fiber during activity have been directed along the lines indicated by what is known of muscle metabolism. In a muscle during contraction heat is produced, the substance of the muscle shows an acid reaction, and among the products formed carbon dioxid gas is perhaps the most prominent. Efforts to show similar reactions in stimulated nerves have been unsuccessful. Rolleston* investigated the question of heat production with the aid of a delicate bolometer capable of indicating a difference of temperature of 5~jot°C. The frog's sciatic was used, but no increase in tem- perature during stimulation could be demonstrated. No change in reaction can be obtained by means of the usual indicators for acidity. Waller has given some experiments to show that carbon dioxid is produced during activity, but they are far from being con- clusive. His line of argument is as follows : He has found that the action current of a nerve that is being stimulated is increased by the presence of very slight amounts of carbon dioxid, higher percentages causing again naturally a decrease. This reaction for the presence of carbon dioxid is apparently a very delicate one. When now a normal nerve is stimulated its action current after some minutes of tetanic stimulation is increased in the same way as would happen if a little carbon dioxid was passed over it. He con- siders that this temporary increase in the action current is due to the formation of carbon dioxid from a functional metabolism. More positive evidence for the occurrence of a nerve metabolism during activity is found in the fact, already alluded to, that oxygen plays a part in maintaining the irritability of nerves. An excised frog's nerve loses its irritability in an atmosphere deprived of oxygen and regains it promptly when oxygen is again supplied. When stimulated in an atmosphere free from oxygen the nerve shows signs of fatigue, while in the presence of oxygen activity is maintained, one may say indefinitely, under continuous stimulation. These facts warrant the belief that oxygen is used by the nerve during activity, and presumably it is used in this as in the other tissues to produce physiological oxidations. An additional fact which points in the same direction is the high value of the temperature coefficient for nerve conduction, which has been referred to above. Bearing these two general considerations in mind, we can hardly escape the conviction that the functional activity of the nerve fiber is connected with a chemical reaction of some kind, most probably a reaction in which some material in the nerve undergoes oxidation. Views as to the Nature of the Nerve Impulse. — The older con- ceptions of the nerve principle, while they varied in detail, were * Rolleston, "Journal of Physiology," 11, 208, 1890. 122 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. based upon the general idea that the nervous system contains a matter of a finer sort than that visible to our senses. This matter was pictured at first as a spirit (animal spirits), and later as a ma- terial comparable to the luminiferous ether or to electricity. Since the discover}- that the nerve impulse travels with a relatively slow velocity and is accompanied by a demonstrable change in the electrical condition of the nerve, two main views regarding its nature have been entertained. Many physiologists conceive the nerve impulse as a progressive wave of chemical change which is started at one end by the stimulus and is then self-propagated along the fiber. The conception in general is represented by the transmission of a spark along a line of gunpowder. The flame applied at one end causes an explosive chemical change, which is then propagated from point to point. The analogy is obviously very incomplete, since in the train of gunpowder the material is entirely consumed, whereas in the nerve an indefinite series of impulses may be trans- mitted and with a strength varying with the intensity of the origina- ting stimulus. This general view implies that a disassimilation or catabolism occurs in the nerve, a breaking down of complex material with the liberation of the potential chemical energy; it assumes, in other words, that the wave of chemical change that sweeps along a nerve fiber is similar to the wave of chemical change, contraction wave, that passes over a muscle fiber. As was stated in preceding paragraphs, there is no direct evidence for this view, although the opinion seems to be growing that the conduction of the nerve impulse depends upon an underlying chemical reaction. On this view the electrical change that accompanies the impulse is considered as a sort of by-action, due probably to the formation of electrolytes in the reaction and the subsequent accumulation of negatively charged ions (anions). The second general view of the nature of the nerve impulse assumes that it is a physical or physico-chemical process transmitted along the fiber without involving a metabolism of the living nerve substance. One may find an analogy for such a process in the wave of pressure transmitted through a tube filled with li<|uif the literature upon the nature "i the nerve impulse consult Boruttau, "Zeit. I', allg. Physiologie, " I, I, Sammelreferate, l!>()2; Biedermann, "Ergebnisse der Physiologie, " vol. ii, part ti, L903; Hering, "Zur Theone der Nerventh&tigkeit, " L899; Gotch, Scliiifer's "Text-book of Physiology," vol. ii, L900. NATURE OF THE NERVE IMPULSE. 125 one could successfully attach the optic nerve to the ear and the auditory nerve to the retina then we should see the thunder and hear the lightning. The alternative theory supposes that nerve impulses are not identical in different fibers, but vary in quality as well as intensity, and that the specific energies of the various fibers depend in part at least on the character of the impulses that they transmit. On this theory one might speak of visual impulses in the optic nerves as something different in kind from the auditory impulses in the auditory fibers. With our present methods of investigation the question is one that can not be definitely decided by experimental investigation; most of the discussion turns upon the applicability of the doctrine to the explanation of various conscious reactions of the sensory nerves. So far as experimental work has been carried out on efferent nerves, it is undoubtedly in favor of the identity theory. The action current is similar in all nerves examined; the reactions to artificial stimuli are essentially similar. Moreover, nerves of one kind may be sutured to nerves of another kind, and, after re- generation has taken place, the reactions are found to be deter- mined solely by the place of ending (see p. 82). The Nutritive Relations of the Nerve Fiber and Nerve Cell. ■ — In recent times in accordance with the so-called neuron doctrine (see p. 131) every axis cylinder has been considered as a process of a nerve cell, and therefore as a part, morphologically speaking, of that cell. However this may be, there is excellent experimental evidence to show that the physiological integrity of the axis cylinder depends upon its connection with its corresponding nerve cell. This view dates from the interesting work of Waller,* who showed that if a nerve be severed the peripheral stump, containing the axis cyl- inders that are cut off from the cells, will degenerate in a few days. The process of degeneration brought about in this way is known as secondary or Wallerian degeneration. The central stump, on the contrary, remains intact, except for a short region immediately contiguous to the wound, for a relatively long period, extending perhaps over years. Waller, therefore, spoke of the nerve cells as forming the nutritive centers for the nerve fibers, and this belief is generally accepted. In what way the cell regulates the nutrition of the nerve fiber throughout its whole length is unknown. Some of the cells in the lumbar spinal cord, for instance, give rise to fibers of the sciatic nerve which may extend as far as the foot, and yet throughout their whole length the nutritive processes in these fibers are dependent on influences of an unknown kind, emanating from * Waller, "Miiller's Archiv," 1852, p. 392; and "Comptes rend us de l'Acad. de la Science," vol. xxxiv., 1S52. 126 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. the nerve cells to which they are joined. These influences may consist simply in the effect of constant activity; that is, in the conduction of nerve impulses, or there may be some kind of an actual transferal of material. This latter idea is supported by the interesting fact, which we owe to Meyer, that tetanus and diph- theria toxins may be transmitted to the central nervous system by way of the axis cylinders of the nerve fibers. By means of his method Waller investigated the location of the nutritive centers for the motor and sensory fibers of the spinal nerves. If an anterior root is cut the peripheral ends of the motor fibers degenerate throughout the length of the nerve, while the fibers in the stump attached to the cord remain intact; hence the nutritive centers for the motor fibers must lie in the cord itself. Subsequent histo- logical work has corroborated this conclusion and shown that the motor fibers of the spinal nerves take their origin from nerve cells lying in the anterior horn of gray matter in the cord, the so-called motor or anterior root cells. If the posterior root is cut between the ganglion and the cord, the stump attached to the cord degener- ates; that attached to the ganglion remains intact, and there is no degeneration in the nerve peripheral to the ganglion (Fig. 55). If, Fig. 55. — Diagram to show the direction of degeneration on section of the anterior and the posterior root, respectively. The degenerated portion is represented in black. however, this root is severed peripherally to the ganglion degenera- tion takes place only in the spinal nerve beyond the ganglion. The nutritive '•enter, therefore, for the sensory fibers must lie in the pos- terior root ganglion, and not in the cord. This conclusion has also been abundantly corroborated by histological work. It is known that the sensory fibers arise from the nerve cells in these ganglia. By the same means it. has been shown that the motor libers in the crania] nerves arise from nerve cells (nuclei of origin) situated in the brain, while the sensory fibers of the same nerves, with the exception of the olfactory and optic nerves which form special arise from sensory ganglia lying outside the nervous axis, NATURE OF THE NERVE IMPULSE. 127 such, for instance, as the spiral ganglia of the cochlear nerve, or the ganglion semihmare (Gasserian ganglion) of the fifth cranial nerve. Nerve Degeneration and Regeneration. — When a nerve trunk is cut or is killed at any point by crushing, heating, or other means all the fibers peripheral to the point of injury undergo de- generation. This is an incontestable fact, and it is important to bear in mind the fact that the definite changes included under the term degeneration are exhibited only by living fibers. A dead nerve or the nerves in a dead animal show no such changes.* The older physiologists thought that if the severed ends of the nerves were brought together by sutures they might unite by first intention without degeneration in the peripheral end. We know now that this degeneration is inevitable once the living continuity of the fibers has been interrupted in any way. Any functional union that may occur is a slow process involving an act of regeneration of the fibers in the peripheral stump. The time required for the degeneration differs somewhat for the different kinds of fibers found in the animal body. In the dog and in other mammalia the degeneration begins in a few (four) days; in the frog it may require from thirty to one hundred and forty days, depending upon the season of the year, although if the frog is kept at a high temperature (30° C.) degeneration may proceed as rapidly as in the mammal. In the dog it proceeds so quickly that the process seems to be simultaneous throughout the whole peripheral stump, while in the frog, and, according to Bethe, in the rabbit, it can be seen clearly that the degenerative changes begin at the wound and progress peripherally. The fibers break up into ellipsoidal segments of myelin, each containing a piece of the axis cylinder, and these segments in turn fragment very irregu- larly into smaller pieces which eventually are absorbed f (Fig. 56). The central stump whose fibers are still connected with the nerve cells undergoes a similar degeneration in the area immediately contiguous to the wound, but the degenerative processes extend for only a short distance over an area covering a few internodal segments. Although the central ends of the fibers remain sub- stantially intact, it is interesting to find that the nerve cells from which they originate undergo distinct changes, which show that they are profoundly affected by the interruption of their norma) connections (see p. 129). In the peripheral end the process of regeneration begins almost simultaneously with the degenerative * See Van Gehuchtea, "Le Nevraxe," 1905, vii., 203. t See Howell and Huber, "Journal of Physiology," 13, 335, 1892; also Mott and Halliburton, "Proceedings Royal Society," 1906, B. lxxviii., 259, and Cajal, "Trabajos del laboratorio de investigaciones biologicas (Univ. of Madrid)," vol. 4, 119, 1906. 12S THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. changes; the two proceeding, as it were, hand in hand. The regen- eration is due to the activity of the nuclei of the neurilemmal sheath. These nuclei begin to multiply and to form around them a layer of protoplasm, so that as the fragments of the old fiber disappear their place is taken by numerous nuclei and their surrounding cytoplasm. Eventually there is formed in this way a continuous strand of protoplasm with man}' nuclei, and the fiber thus produced, which has no resemblance in structure to a normal nerve fiber, is described by some authors as an "embryonic fiber"; by others as a "band fiber"' (Fig. 57). In the adult animal the process of regeneration stops at this point unless an anatomical connection is established with the central stump, and, indeed, such a connection is usually established unless special means are taken to prevent it. The central and peripheral stumps find each other in a way that is often remarkable, the union being guided doubtless by intervening connective tissue. Forsmanns * has emphasized this peculiar attraction, as it were, be- tween the peripheral and the central ends, giving some reason to believe that it is a case of ehemotaxis or chemotropism. When the ends of the nerves were given very unusual positions by means of collodium tubes into which they were inserted they managed to " find" each other. Moreover, he states that a central stump, if given an equal opportunity to grow into two collo- dium tul >es, one containing liver and the other brain tissue, will chose the latter, a fact which would indicate some underlying chemical attraction or affinity in nerve tissue for nerve tissue. A directive influence of this kind depending upon some property connected with chemical relationship is desig- nated as " ehemotaxis." if the central and peripheral stumps are brought together by suture or grow together in any way, then, under the influence of the central end, the "band fiber" gradually becomes transformed into a normal nerve fiber, with myelin sheath and axis cylinder (Fig. 58). It is possible that this result is (\ue to local processes in the band fiber stimulated by nutritive influences of some kind from the central stump, but more probably there is an actual down- growth of the axis cylinders from the central ends. In support of this hitter view, it may be said thai the outgrowth of the new axis cylinders from the old ones present in the libers of the central stump has been followed more or less successfully by a number of histologists. From .'] practical standpoint it- re interesting to note that this influence of the central stump may be exerted months or even years after the injury to i he aerve. The peripheral si ump after reaching the stage <>f " band fibers " i ready, &t ii were, for the influence of the central end, and canes are onprecord in which a secondary Buture was made a Long time after the original injury, with the resuh thai functional activity was restored to the nerve. Bethef has thrown some doubl upon this view, for he has shown appar- ently thai in young mammals (eight days to eighl weeks) the regeneration of * Forsmanns, "Zeigler's Beitrage," 27, 216, L902. f Bethe, "Allgemeine Anat. u. Physiologie des Nervensystems, " 190.3. Fig. 56. — Histology of a degenerating nerve fiber. Fig. 57. — Embryonic fibers in a regenerating nerve Fig. 58. — A newly developed fiber in a regenerating nerve fiber, NATURE OF THE NERVE IMPULSE. 129 the fibers in the peripheral stump does not stop at the stage of " band fibers," but progresses until perfectly normal nerve fibers are produced, even though no connection is made with the central stump. It should be added, however, that the fibers so formed do not persist indefinitely unless they become con- nected with the central stump. If this connection fails to take place, the newly formed fibers will degenerate after an interval of some months. Still, the fact, if true, that in the young fiber the regeneration is complete seems to indicate definitely that the axis cylinder may arise independently of the fibers in the central stump. Whether or not Bethe's observations upon the autoregeneration of the axis cylinders in the severed nerves of young animals can be accepted is at present uncertain, but the balance of evidence seems to indicate that what he took for autoregenerated fibers were really fibers which grew into the degenerated trunk from the surrounding tissue. (For discussion with refer- ences, see Barker, "Journal of the American Medical Assoc," 1906, Stefan- owska, "Journal de Neurologie," 1906, Nos. 16-19, and Halliburton, "British Med. Journal," May 11, 1907.) Degenerative Changes in the Neuron on the Central Side of the Lesion. — According to the Wallerian law of degeneration, as originally stated, the nerve fiber on the central side of the injury and the nerve cell itself do not undergo any change. As a matter of fact, the central stump immediately contiguous to the lesion undergoes typical degeneration and regeneration similar to that described for the fibers of the peripheral stump. The immediate degenerative changes in the fibers in the central stump were supposed to extend back only to the first node of Ranvier, — to affect, there- fore, only the internodal segment actually injured. Later it was found that the degeneration may extend back over a distance of several internodal segments. This limited degeneration on the central side must be considered as traumatic, — that is, it involves only those portions directly injured by the lesion. The central end of the fiber in general was supposed to remain intact as long as its cell of origin was normal. It was thought at first that after simple section of a nerve trunk, in amputation, for instance, the nerve cells and central stumps remain normal throughout the life of the individual. Dickinson, however, in 1869 * showed that in amputations of long standing the motor cells in the anterior horn of the cord decrease in number and the fibers in the central stump become atrophied. This observation has been corroborated by other observers, and it is now believed that after section of a nerve chronic degenerative changes ensue in the course of time in the central fibers and their cells, resulting in their permanent atrophy. We have, in such cases, what has been called an atrophy from disuse. A fact that has been discovered more recently and that is perhaps of more importance is that the nerve cells do undergo certain definite although usually temporary changes immediately after the section of the nerve fibers arising from them. It has been shown that when a nerve is cut the corresponding cells of origin * " Journakof Anatomy and Physiology," 3, 176, 1869. 9 130 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF MUSCLE AND NERVE. may show distinct histological changes within the first twenty-four hours. These changes consist in a circumscribed destruction of the chromatin material in the cells (chromatolysisV, which in a short time extends over the whole cell, so that the primary staining power of the cell is lost (condition of achromatosis) (see Fig. 63). The cell also becomes swollen and the nucleus may assume an excentric position. These retrogressive changes continue for a certain period (about eighteen days). After reaching their maxi- mum of intensity the cells usually undergo a process of restitution and regain their normal appearance, although in some cases the degeneration is permanent. According to other observers a number of the cells in the spinal cord and spinal ganglia undergo simple atrophy after section of their corresponding nerves, and some of the nerve fibers in the central stumps may also show atrophy, while others undergo a genuine degeneration, which, however, comes on much later than in the peripheral stumps. It seems evident that the behavior of the cells and fibers on the central side of the section is not uniform; atrophy rather than degeneration is the change that is prominent, and this atrophy in some neurons occurs early, while in others it is apparent only after a long interval of time. An explanation of this variation in the reaction of the nerve cells and their disconnected central stumps cannot yet be given. On the peripheral side of the section, as stated above, the de- generative changes are complete and affect all of the fibers.* *Nissl, 'Allgemeine Zeitschrift f. Psychiatrie, " 48, 197, 1892. Also Bethe, loc. cit., and Hanson, "Retrograde Degeneration in t he Spinal Nerves," The Journal of Comparative Neurology and Psychology, 1906, xvi., 265. SECTION II. THE PHYSIOLOGY OF THE CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. CHAPTER VI. STRUCTURE AND GENERAL PROPERTIES OF THE NERVE CELL. The Neuron Doctrine. — Since the last decade of the nineteenth century the physiology of the nervous system has been treated from the standpoint of the neuron. According to this point of view, the entire nervous system is made up of a series of units, the neurons, which are not anatomically continuous with each other, but communicate by contact only. It has been taught also that each neuron represents from an anatomical and physiological standpoint a single nerve cell. The typical neuron consists of a cell body with short, branching processes, the dendrites, and a single axis cylinder process, the axon or axite, which becomes a nerve fiber, acquiring its myelin sheath at some distance from the cell. According to this view, the peripheral nerve fibers are simpry long processes from nerve cells. Within the central nervous system each neuron connects with others according to a certain schema. The axon of each neuron ends in a more or less branched " terminal arborization," forming a sort of end-plate which lies in contact with the dendrites of another neuron, or in some cases with the body of the cell itself, the essentially modern point of view being that where the terminal arborization of the axon meets the dendrites or body of another neuron the communication is by contact, the neurons being anatomically independent units. It is usually ac- cepted also as a part of the neuron doctrine that the conduction of a nerve impulse through a neuron is always in one direction, that the dendrites are receiving organs, so to speak, receiving a stimulus or impulse from the axon of another unit and conveying this impulse toward the cell body, while the axon is a discharging process through which an impulse is sent out from the cell to reach another neuron or a cell of some other tissue. The neuron, so 131 132 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. far as conduction is concerned, shows a definite polarity, the con- duction in the dendrites being cellulipetal, in the axons, cellulifugal. The neuron doctrine, so far as the name at least is concerned, dates from a general paper by Waldeyer,* in which the newer work up to that time was summarized. The main facts upon which the conception rests were furnished by His (lSSG"), to whom we owe the generally accepted belief that the nerve fiber (axis cylinder) is an outgrowth from the cell, and secondly by Golgi, Cajal, and a host of other workers, who, by means of the new method of Golgi, demonstrated the wealth of branches of the nerve cells, particularly of the dendrite?, and the mode of connection of one nerve unit with another. The view that these units are anatomically independent and on the embryological i — Motor cell, anterior horn of gray matter of cord. From human fetus (Lenhoa- sek): * marks the axon; the other branches are dendrite ■ aide are derived each from a single epiblastic cell (neuroblast) has proved acceptable and mos1 helpful; but the validity of this hypothesis has been called into question from time to time. As was stated on p. L28, Bethe has claimed thai in young animals the nuclei of the neurilemma] sheath may regenerate :i new nerve fiber containing axis cylinder and myelin Bheath, and this fact, if true, .-it once bring into que tion the hitherto accepted belief that the axis cylinder can I"- formed only as an outgrowth from a nerve cell. Bome histologistf Apathy, Bethe, Nissl have also attacked the most fundamental feature of the neuron doctrine the view, namely, that each neuron represents an independent anatomical element. These authors contend thai the neurofibrile of the axi cylinder pass through the nerve cells and enter by way of a network into direct connection with the neurofibrils *"Deut. med. Wochenschrift, " A891, p. 50. PROPERTIES OF THE NERVE CELL. 133 of other neurons (see Fig. 64). The neurofibrils form a continuum through which nerve impulses pass without a break from neuron to neuron. Ac- cording to this conception, the ganglion cells play no direct part in the con- duction of the impulse from one part of the nervous system to another; the neurofibrils alone, and the intracellular and pericellular networks with which they connect, form the conducting paths that are everywhere in con- tinuity. In the explanation given below of the activities of the nervous system, the author, following the usual custom, makes use of the neuron doctrine, since it is at present impossible to say whether or not the newer views of the continuum of neurofibrils will be corroborated. While the physiological facts remain the same whichever view prevails, there can be no doubt that the point of view of the physiologist would be greatly changed if the present simple conception of a series of neurons of a definite polarity as regards conduction were replaced by the more complex schema of inde- pendent neurofibrils and a central reticulum in which a basis for polarity and definite paths of conduction is lacking.* The Varieties of Neurons. — The neurons differ greatly in size, shape, and internal structure, and it is impossible to classify them with entire success from either a physiological or an anatomical standpoint. Neglecting the unusual forms whose occurrence is limited and whose structure is perhaps incompletely known, there are three distinct types whose form and structure throw some light on their functional significance: I. The bipolar cells. This cell is found in the dorsal root gan- glia of the spinal nerves and in the ganglia attached to the sensory fibers of the cranial nerves, the ganglion semilunare (Gasserian) for the fifth cranial, the g. geniculi for the seventh, the g. vestibu- lare and g. spirale for the eighth, the g. superius and g. petrosum for the ninth, the g. jugulare and g. nodosum for the tenth. The typical cell of this group is found in the dorsal root ganglia. In the adult the two processes arise as one, so that the cell seems to be unipolar, but at some distance from the cell this process divides in T, one branch passing into the spinal cord via the posterior root, the other entering the spinal nerve as a sensory nerve fiber to be distributed to some sensory surface. Both processes become medullated and form typical nerve fibers. That these apparently unipolar cells are really bipolar is shown not only by this division into two distinct fibers, but also by a study of their development in the embryo. In early embryonic life the two processes arise from different poles of the cell, and later become fused into an ap- parently simple process (Fig. 60). The striking characteristics of this cell, therefore, are that it gives rise to two nerve fibers, and that it possesses no dendritic processes. On the physiological side these cells might be designated as sensory cells, since they appear to be associated always with sensory nerve fibers. * For discussion, see Barker, "Journal of the American Medical Associa- tion," 1906, and Retzius, "Proceedings of the Royal Society," 1908, B. vol. lxxx., 414. 134 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. The nerve cells found in the sensory ganglia exhibit, as a matter of fact, a number of different types, some of which possess short dendritic processes. These histological variations cannot as yet be given a physiological signifi- cance, but their occurrence certainly seems to indicate a possibility that the sensory ganglia may have a much more varied physiological activity than has been attributed to them heretofore. For a recent description of these ganglia and a classification of their cells under eight different types consult Oajal in Ergebnisse der Anat. u. Entwickelungsgeschichte, vol. xvi., 1906. So far as the sensory fibers of the spinal and cranial nerves are concerned, it is worth noting also that all of them arise from cells lying outside the main axis of the central nervous system. It has been a question whether the sensory impulses brought to the ganglion cells through the peripheral process (sensory Via. 60. — Bipolar cells in the posterior root ganglion. Section through spinal pan- glion of newborn mouse (Lenhossek) : a, The spinal ganglion; b, the spinal cord; C, the posterior, d, the anterior root. fiber) passes into the body of the cell before going on to the eord or brain, or whether at the junction of the two processes it -imply passes on directly to the cord. According to the histological structure there is no apparent reason why an impulse should not pass directly from tin; peripheral to the ('(Mitral process at the junction, but whether or not this really occurs and the relation of the ganglion cell to the conducting' path are questions thai musl lie left- unsettled at present. II. The multipolar cells. The processes of these colls fall into two groups: the short and branching dendrites with an inner structure resembling that of the cell body, and the axon or axis cylinder process ( Fig. 59). According to the structure of this last, processl , t his t ype may be classified under t wo heads: ( rolgi cells of t he firsl and t he second type. The cells of t lie first, type are charac- PROPERTIES OF THE NERVE CELL. 135 terized by the fact that the axon leaves the central gray matter and becomes a nerve fiber. This nerve fiber within the central nervous system may give off numerous coilateralsl, each of which ends in a terminal arborization. By this means the neurons of this type may be brought into physiological connection with a number of other neurons. This kind of nerve cell is frequently described as the typical nerve cell. Golgi supposed that it rep- resents the motor type of cell, and this view is, in a measure, borne out by sub- sequent investigation. The distinctly motor cells of the central nervous system — such, for instance, as the pyramidal cells of the cere- bral cortex, the anterior horn cells of the spinal cord, the Pur kin je cells of the cere- bellum— all belong to this - _ — — a-n w Fig. 61. — Golgi cell (second type). The axon, a, divides into a number of fine branches. — (From Obersteiner, after Andriezen.) Fig. 62. — Normal anterior horn cell (Warrington), showing the Nissl granules in the cell and dendrites: a, The axon. type. But within the nerve axis most of the conduction from neuron to neuron, along sensory as well as motor paths, is made with the aid of such structures, the dendrites being the receptive or sensory organ and the axon the motor apparatus. The Golgi cells of the second type (Fig. 61) are relatively less numerous and important. They are characterized by the fact lo6 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. that the axon process instead of forming a nerve fiber splits into a great number of branches within the gray matter. Assuming that in such cells the distinction between the axon and the den- drites is well made and that as in the other type the dendrites form the receiving and the axon the discharging apparatus, these cells would seem to have a distributive function. The impulse that they receive may be transmitted to one or many neurons. They are sometimes spoken of as intermediate or association cells. Internal Structure of the Nerve Cell. — Within the body of the nerve cell itself the striking features of physiological signifi- cance are, first, the arrangement of the neurofibrils, and, second, the .--»*. «W?,N .. j Fig. 03. — Anterior horn cell fourteen days after section of the anterior root (Warring- ton) : To .show the change in the nucleus and the Nissl granules, beginning chromatolysis. presence of a material in the form of granules, rods, or masses which stains readily with the basic anilin dyes, such as methylene blue, thionin, or toluidin blue. This latter substance is spoken of as the " chromophile substance," tigroid, or more frequently as Nissl's granules, after the histologist who first studied it success- fully. These masses or granules are found in the dendrites as well as in the cell, but are absent from the axon (see Tig. G2). Little is known of their composition or significance1, but their presence or ab- sence is in many cases characteristic of the physiological condition of the cell. After lesions or injuries of the neuron the material may become dissolved and diffused through the cell or may decrease in amount or disappear, and it seems probable, therefore, that it repre- sents a store of nutritive material (Fig. 63). The non-staining material Of the cell, according to most recent observers, contains neurofibrils which are continued out into the processes, dendrites as well a axons. These fibrils may be regarded as the conducting Structure along which passes the nerve impulse. The arrangement, of these fibrils within the cell is not completely known, the results obtained varying with the methods employed. A matter of far- PROPERTIES OP THE NERVE CELL. 137 reaching importance on the physiological side is the question of the existence of an extracellular nervous network. Most recent histologists agree in the belief that there is a delicate network surrounding the cells and their protoplasmic processes. This pericellular net or Golgi's net is claimed by some to be a ner- vous structure connecting with the neurofibrils inside the cell and forming not only a bond of union between the neurons, but possibly also an important intercellular nervous structure that may play an important role in the functions of the nerve centers. This view is represented schematically in Fig. 64. According to others, this network around and outside the cells is a supporting tissue simply that takes no part in the activity of the nerve units. Fig. 64. — Bethe's schema to indicate the connections of the pericellular network: Rz, A sensory cell in the posterior root ganglion ; the fibrils in the branch that runs to the cord are indicated as connecting directly with the pericellular network of the motor cells, Gz. General Physiology of the Nerve Cell. — Modern physiologists have considered the cell body of the neuron, including the den- drites, as the source of the energy displayed by the nervous system, and it has been assumed that this energy arises from chemical changes in the nerve cell, as the energy liberated by the muscle arises from the chemical changes in its substance. It would follow from this standpoint that evidences of chemical activity should be obtained from the cells and that these elements should exhibit the phenomenon of fatigue. Regarding this latter point, it is believed in physiology that the nerve cells do show fatigue. The nerve centers fatigue as the result of continuous activity, as is evident from our personal experience in prolonged intellectual or emo- tional activity and as is implied in the necessity of sleep for re- cuperation and the rapidity with which functional activity is lost 138 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. on withdrawal of the blood supply. Objectively, also, it has been shown in the ergographic experiments (see p. 49) that the well- known fatigue of the neuromuscular apparatus possibly affects the nerve centers as well as the muscle. Assuming that the nerve cells are the effective agent in the nerve centers, such facts indicate that they are susceptible to fatigue. But we have no very direct- proof that this property is possessed universally by the nerve cells nor any indication of the probable differences in this regard shown by nerve cells in different parts of the central nervous system. It seems probable that under normal conditions — that is, under the influence of what we may call minimal stimuli — some portions of the nerve centers remain in more or less constant activity during the day without showing a marked degree of fatigue, just as our muscles remain in a more or less continuous state of tonic con- traction throughout the waking period at least. Doubtless when the stimulation is stronger the fatigue is more marked, because the processes of repair in the nerve centers can not then keep pace with the processes of consumption of material. Evidence of a probable chemical change in the nerve cells during activity is found also in the readiness with which the gray matter of the nervous system takes on an acid reaction.* In the fresh resting state it is probably alkaline or neutral, but after death it quickly shows an acid reaction, due, it is said, to the production of lactic acid. Its resemblance to the muscle in this respect leads to the inference that in functional activity acid is also produced. Mosso states that in the brain increased mental activity is accompanied by a rise in the temperature of the brain. f His experiments were made upon individuals with an opening in the skull through which a delicate thermometer could be inserted so as to lie in contact with brain. So also the facts briefly mentioned in regard to the Nissl granules give some corroborative evidence that the activity of the nervous system is accompanied by and probably caused by a chemical change within the cells, since the excessive activity of the nerve fells seems to be accompanied by some change in these granules, and in abnormal conditions associated with loss of func- tional activity (lie granules undergo chromatolysis,- — that is, the}' are disintegrated and dissolved. < >bvious histological changes which imply, of course, a change in chemical si ructure, have been observed by a number of investigators.} All seem to agree that activity of tin- tissue, whether normal or induced by artificial stimulation, may cause visible changes in the appearance of the cell and its * Langendorff, "Centralbl. f. 'I. med. VViss.," 1886. Bee also Halliburton, 'The Croonian Lecture on 'I \><- Chemical Side of Nervous Activity," 1901. \]., ■, Dii Temperatui des Gehirns," L894. pecially Hodge, "Journal of Morphology," 7, !).r», 1S".)2, and 9, 1, 1894. PROPERTIES OF THE NERVE CELL. 139 nucleus. Activity within normal limits may cause an increase in the size of the cell together with a diminution in the stainable (Nissl) substance, and excessive activity a diminution in size of the cell and the nucleus, the formation of vacuoles in the cell body, and a marked effect upon the stainable material. Hodge has shown that in birds, for instance, the spinal ganglion cells of a swallow killed at nightfall after a day of activity exhibit a marked loss of substance as compared with similar cells from an animal killed in the early morning (Fig. 65). It must be remembered, however, that our knowledge of the nature of the chemical changes that occur in the cell during activity is very meager. Presumably carbon dioxid and lactic acid are Fig. 65. — Spinal ganglion cells from English sparrows, to show the daily variation in the appearance of the cells due to normal activity: A. Appearance of cells at the end of an active day; B, appearance of cells in the morning after a night's rest. The cytoplasm is filled with clear, lenticular masses, which are much more evident in the rested cells than in those fatigued. — (Hodge.) formed as in muscle, and we know that oxygen is consumed. Enough is known perhaps to justify the general view that the energy exhibited by the nervous system is derived, in the long run, from a metabolism of material in the nerve cells, a metabolism which consists essentially in the splitting and oxidation of the complex substances in the protoplasm of the cell. Summation of the Effects of Stimuli. — In a muscle a series of stimuli will cause a greater amount of shortening than can be obtained from a single stimulus of the same strength. In this case the effects of the stimuli are summated, one contraction taking place on top of another, or to put it in another way, the muscle while in a condition of contraction from one stimulus is made to 140 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. contract still more by the following stimulus. In the nerve fiber such a phenomenon has not been demonstrated. The strength of the nerve impulse can be determined only by means of the effect on the end-organ,— c. g., the muscle, — in which case the properties of the end-organ must be taken into account, or by the aid of the electrical response. Now, when a nerve is stimulated so rapidly that the second stimulus falls into the nerve before the electrical change clue to the first stimulus has passed off, the second stimulus, instead of adding its effect to that of the first, simply has no effect at all; it finds the nerve unirritable.* According to this result, we should expe^ that a summation of the effects of rapidly following stimuli is not possible in the case of the nerve fiber. In the nerve cell, on the contrary, it is usually taught that the power of summation is a characteristic property, although it may be said that the proofs for this belief are not very direct. It is pointed out that, while a single stimulus applied to a sensory nerve ma}' be ineffective in producing a reflex response from the central nervous system, a series of such stimuli will call forth a reaction. In this case it is assumed that the effects of the suc- ceeding stimuli are summated within the nerve cells through which the reflex takes place, and, generally speaking, it is assumed in physiology that the nerve centers are adapted by their power of summation to respond to a series of stimuli or to continuous stimu- lation. The best examples of this kind of action are obtained perhaps from sensory nerves, in which case we judge of the intensity of the cell activity by the concomitant sensation, or by a reflex response. Response of the Nerve Cell to Varying Rates of Stimula- tion.— The various parts of the neuromuscular apparatus — namely, the nerve cell, the nerve fiber, and the muscle fiber — have different degrees of responsiveness to repeated stimuli, and this responsiveness varies, moreover, for the different kinds of mus- cles and of nerve fibers, and, probably for the different kinds of nerve eells. The motor cells of the brain and cord discharge their impulses under normal stimulation at a certain rhythm which was formerly supposed to average about 10 per second, but is now estimated as varying between certain wide limits, perhaps from 40 to 100 per second (p. 46). For any particular group of nerve cells the evidence indicates that it has a practically constanl rate whatever may be the intensity of the stimulus and, indeed, when artificial stimulation is used and the rate is varied, the evidence that we have so far appears to show that the nerve cells do not discharge in a one to one correspondence with the rate of stimulation, as is the case, within limits, for muscle and nerve fibers. On the contrary, under such circum- • Gotch and Burch, "Journal of Physiology," 24, 410, L899. PROPERTIES OP THE NERVE CELL, 141 stances the discharge from the nerve cells takes place in a rhythm characteristic of the cells and independent of that of the stimulation.* From this point of view we must look upon the nerve cells as possessing fundamentally a rhythmic activity, as in the case of the heart. The Refractory Period of the Nerve Cell. — The peculiar rhythm of the active nerve cell just referred to in the para- graph above is explained most satisfactorily by an assumption first used in connection with the rhythmical beat of the heart. As will be explained more fully in the section on the physiology of the heart, it has been found that after the contraction of the heart begins it is unirritable to artificial stimuli, and that its irritability is recovered during the period of rest, — the diastole. The heart has, therefore, alternate periods of irritability and unirritability. The latter phase, the condition in which the heart muscle will not respond to stimulation, is known as the refractory period, or refractory phase. A similar conception has been applied to the nerve cell, to explain its rhythmic activity. It has been applied also to a phenomenon of a somewhat different order, namely, to a loss of irritability exhibited by some of the spinal centers for a brief period after reflex excitation (Sherrington). Reasoning from analogy with the muscle, we might suppose that this temporary loss of irritability is due to some product of the chemical reaction that is assumed to underlie nervous activity. Using this terminology, it is probable that the cells in different parts of the nervous system may have different refractory periods. In the case of the normal nerve fiber (see p. 120) it will be recalled that the refractory period is very brief — say, 0.006 sec. — but varies with the condition of the fiber, since in the narcotized fiber it may be as much as 0.1 sec. * Horsley and Schafer, "Journal of Physiology/' 7, 96, 1886. CHAPTER VII. REFLEX ACTIONS. Definition and Historical. — By a reflex action we mean the involuntary production of activity in some peripheral tissue through the efferent nerve fibers connected with it in consequence of a stimulation of afferent nerve fibers. The conversion of the sensory or afferent impulse into a motor or efferent impulse is effected in the nerve centers, and may be totally unconscious as well as invol- untary,— for instance, the emptying of the gall-bladder during digestion, or it may be accompanied by consciousness of the act, as, for example, in the winking reflex when the eye is touched. The application of the term reflex to such acts seems to have been made first by Descartes* (1649), on the analogy of the reflection of light, the sensory effect in these cases being reflected back, so to speak, as a motor effect. The attention of the early physiologists was directed to these involuntary movements and many instances were collected, both in man and the lower animals. Their invol- untary character was emphasized by the discovery that similar movements arc given by decapitated animals, — frogs, eels, etc. Some of the earlier physiologists thought that the reflex might occur in the anastomoses of the nerve trunks, but a convincing proof that the central nervous system is the place of reflection was given by Whytt (1751). He showed that in a decapitated frog the reflex movements are abolished if the spinal cord is destroyed. Modem interesl in the subject was excited by the numerous works of Marshal] Hall (1832-57), who contributed a number of new facts with regard to such acts, and formulated a view, not now accepted, I hat t hese reflexes are mediated by a special set of fibers - i he excitomotor fillers. In describing reflexes the older physiologists had in mind only reflex movements, but at the present time we recognize that the reflex act mayaffecl not only the muscles, voluntary, involuntary, and cardiac, hut also 1 he glands. We have to deal with rellex Becretions as well as reflex movements. The Reflex Arc. — It is implied in the definition of a reflex thai both sensory and motor paths are concerned in the act. Ac- * Sec Eckhard, " Geschichte der Entwickelune der Lehre von den Eteflex- eiBcheinungen," "Beitr&ge zur Anatomie u. Physiologie," Giessen, 1881, vol. ix. I IL' REFLEX ACTIONS. 143 cording to the neuron theory, therefore, the simplest reflex arc must consist of two neurons: the sensory neuron, whose cell body lies in the sensory ganglia of the posterior roots or of the cranial nerves, and a motor neuron, whose nerve cell lies in the anterior horn of gray matter of the cord or in the motor nucleus of a cranial nerve. The reflex arc for the spinal cord is reprensented in Fig. 66. The arc may, however, be more complex. The sensory fibers entering through the posterior roots may pass upward through the entire length of the cord to end in the medulla, and on the way give off a number of collaterals as is represented in Fig. 67, or they may make connections with intermediate cells which, in turn, are con- nected with one or more motor neurons (Fig. 68). According Fig. 66. — Schema to show the connection between the neuron of the posterior root and the neuron of the anterior root, — the reflex arc. to these schemata, one sensory fiber may establish reflex connections with a number of different motor fibers, or, a fact which must be borne in mind in studying some of the well-known reflex activities of the cord and medulla especially, a sensory fiber carrying an impulse which eventually reaches the cortex of the cerebrum and gives rise to a conscious sensation may, by means of its collaterals, connect with motor nuclei in the cord or medulla and thus at the same time give origin to involuntary and even unconscious re- flexes. Painful stimulation of the skin, for example, may give us a conscious sensation of pain and at the same time reflexly stimulate the vasomotor center and cause a constriction of the small arteries. The fact that in this case two distinct events occur does not necessitate the assumption that the impulses from the 144 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTKAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. skin are carried to the cord by two different varieties of fibers. It may well be that one variety of sensory neuron, the so-called pain fibers, effects both results, because of the opportunities in the cord for connections with different groups of nerve cells. The Reflex Frog. — The motor reflexes from the spinal cord can be studied most successfully upon a frog in which the brain has been destroyed or whose head has been cut off. After such an operation the animal may for a time suffer from shock, but a vigorous animal will usually recover and after some hours will I'ig. 07. — Kolliker's schema to show the -lireet reflex urc. It .shows the pos- terior tool IiIkt (black) entering the oord, dividing in Y. and connecting with motor i-'il (red) by means of collater- als. Fin. OS. — Kolliker's schema to show the reflex are with intercal- ated tract cells. Posterior root liber, black; intercalated tract cell, blue; motor cells, red. exhibit reflex movements that are most interesting. The funda- mental characteristics of reflex movements in their relations to the place, intensity, and quality of the stimulus can be studied with more ease upon an animal whose cord is thus severed from the brain than upon a normal animal. In the latter case the connec- tion- in the nervous System are more complex and the relictions are therefore less simple and less easily kept constant. Spinal Reflex Movements. The reflex movements obtained from the spinal cord or from ot her parts of thecentral nervous system REFLEX ACTIONS. 145 may be divided into three groups by characteristics that are physio- logically significant. These classes are: (1) Simple reflexes, or those in which a single muscle is affected. The best example of this group is perhaps the winking reflex, in which only the orbic- ularis palpebrarum is concerned. (2) Co-ordinated reflexes, in which a number of muscles react with their contractions so grad- uated as to time and extent as to produce an orderly and useful movement. (3) Convulsive reflexes, such as are seen in spasms, in Avhich a number of muscles — perhaps all the muscles — are con- tracted convulsively, without co-ordination and with the pro- duction of disorderly and useless movements. Of these groups, the co-ordinated reflexes are by far the most interesting. They can be obtained to perfection from the reflex frog. In such an animal no spontaneous movements occur if the sensory surfaces are entirely protected from stimulation. A sudden stimulus, however, of sufficient strength applied to any part of the skin will give a definite and practically invariable response in a movement which has the appearance of an intentional effort to escape from or remove the stimulus. If the toe is pinched the foot is withdrawn — in a gentle manner if the stimulus is light, more rapidly and violently, but still in a co-ordinated fashion, if the stimulus is strong. If the animal is suspended and various spots on its skin are stimulated by the application of bits of paper moistened with dilute acetic acid the animal will make a neat and skillful movement of the corresponding leg to remove the stimulating body. The reactions may be varied in a number of ways, and in all cases the striking features of the reflex response are, first, the seemingly purposeful character of the movement, and, second, the almost mechanical exactness with which a definite stimulus will give a definite response. This definite relationship holds only for sensory stimulation of the external integument, the skin and its organs. It is obvious, in fact, that a muscular response can be effective only for stimuli originating from the external surface. Stimuli from the interior of the body exert their reactions, for the most part, upon the plain musculature and the glands. The convulsive reflexes may be produced by two different means : (1) By very intense sensory stimulation. The reflex response in this case overflows, as it were, into all the motor paths. A variation of this method is seen in the well-known convulsive reaction that follows tickling. In this case the stimulus, although not intense from an objective standpoint, is obviously violent from the standpoint of its effectiveness in sending into the central nervous system a series of maximal sensory impulses. (2) By heightening the irritability of the central nervous system. Upon the reflex frog this effect is obtained most readily by the use of strychnin. A little strychnin injected under the 10 14G PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. skin is soon absorbed and its effect is shown at first by a greater sensitiveness to cutaneous stimulation, the slightest touch to the foot causing its withdrawal. Soon, however, the response, instead of being orderly and adapted to a useful end, becomes convulsive. A mere touch of the skin or a current of air will throw every muscle into contraction, and the extensors being stronger than the flexors the animal's body becomes rigid in extension at every stimulation. The explanation usually given for this result is that the strychnin, acting upon some part of the nerve cells, increases greatly their irritability, so that when a stimulus is sent into the central nervous system along any sensory path from the skin it apparently radiates throughout the cord and acts upon all the motor cells. This latter supposition leads to the interesting con- clusion that all the various motor neurons of the cord must be in physiological connection, either direct or indirect, with all the neurons supplying the cutaneous surface. The further fact that under normal conditions the effect of a given sensory stimulus is manifested only on a limited and practically constant number of the motor neurons seems to imply, therefore, that normally the paths to these neurons are more direct and the resistance, if we may use a somewhat figurative term, is less than that offered by other possible paths. Muscular spasms are observed under a number of pathological conditions, l'<>r instance, in hydrophobia. We arc; at liberty to assume in such cases that the toxins produced by the disease affect the irritability of the cells in much the same way as the strychnin. Theory of Co-ordinated Reflexes. — The purposeful character of the co-ordinated reflexes in the frog gives the impression to the observer of a conscious choice of movements on the part of the brainless animal. Most physiologists, however, are content to see in these reactions only an expression of the automatic activity of a mechanism. It is assumed that the sensory impulses from any part of the skin find, on reaching the cord, that the paths to a certain group of motor neurons are more direct and oiler less resistance than any others. It is along these paths that the reflex will take place, and we may further assume that these paths of leas! resistance, as they have been called, are in part preformed and in pari are laid down by the repeated experiences of the indi- vidual. Thai is, in each animal a definite structure may be sup- po ed to exisl in the cord; each sensory neuron is connected with a group of motor neurons, to some of them more directly than to others, and we may imagine, therefore, a system of reflex apparatuses Or mechanisms which when properly stimulated will react, always in the same way. And, indeed, in spite of the adapted character of the reflexes under consideration their automaton-like regularity i an indication ihat their production is due to a fixed mechanical REFLEX ACTIONS. 147 arrangement. Whether or not the reactions of the nervous system in such cases are accompanied by any degree of consciousness can not be proved or disproved, but the assumption of such an accom- paniment does not seem necessary to explain the reaction. Spinal Reflexes in the Mammals. — Experiments upon the lower mammals, such as the dog, show that co-ordinated reflex move- ments may be obtained from the lower portion of the cord after severance of its connections with the brain. The spinal cord may be severed, for instance, in the thoracic region and the animal be kept alive and in good condition for an indefinite period. In such an animal reflex movements of the hind legs or tail may be obtained readily from slight sensory stimulation of the skin. The knee-jerk and similar so-called deep reflexes are also retained. But it is evident that these movements are not so complete nor so distinctly adapted to a useful end as in the frog. The muscles of the body supplied by the isolated part of the cord retain, however, a normal irritability and exhibit no wasting. In man, on the contrary, it is stated that after complete section of the cord the deep reflexes, such as the knee-jerk, as well as the skin reflexes, are very quickly lost. The muscles undergo wasting and soon lose their irritability.* The monkeys exhibit in this respect a condition that is somewhat intermediate between that of the dog and man. It seems evident from these facts that in the lower animals, like the frog, a much greater degree of independent activity is exhibited by the cord than in the more highly developed animals. According to the degree of development, the control of the muscles is assumed more and more by the higher portions of the nervous system, and the spinal cord becomes less important as a series of reflex centers, its functions being more dependent upon its connections with the higher centers. Dependence of Co-ordinated Reflexes upon the Excitation of the Normal Sensory Endings, — It is an interesting fact that when a nerve trunk is stimulated directly in a reflex frog — the sciatic nerve, for instance — the reflex movements are disorderly and quite unlike those obtained by stimulating the skin. It is said that if the skin be loosened and the nerve twigs arising from it be stimulated, an operation that is quite possible in the frog, the re- sponse is again a disorderly reflex, whereas the same fibers stimu- lated through the skin give an orderly, co-ordinated movement. The difference in response in these cases is probably not due to any peculiarity in the nature of the sensory impulses originating in the nerve endings of the skin, but more likely to a difference in their strength and arrangement. When one stimulates a sensory nerve trunk directly, — the ulnar nerve at the elbow in ourselves, for in- * See Collier, " Brain." 1904, p. 38. 14S PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. stance, — the resulting sensations are markedly different from those obtained by stimulating the skin areas supplied by the same nerve; we have little or no sensations of touch or temperature, only pain and a peculiar tingling in the fingers. In such an experiment the stimulus applied to the trunk affects more or less equally all the contained fibers, whereas in stimulation of the skin itself the effect upon the cutaneous fibers of pressure, temperature, or pain pre- dominates and presumably it is these fibers that normally are con- nected in an efficient way with the reflex machinery in the nerve centers. Reflex Time. — Since nerve centers are involved in a reflex movement, a determination of the total time between the appli- cation of the stimulus and the beginning of the response gives a means of ascertaining the time needed for the processes within the nerve cells. Helmholtz, who first made experiments of this kind, stated that the time required within the nerve centers might be as much as twelve times as great as that estimated for the conduction along the motor and sensory nerves involved in the reflex. Most observers state that the time within the center varies with the strength of the stimulus, being less, the stronger the stimulus. It varies also with the condition of the nerve centers, being lengthened by fatigue and other conditions that depress the irritability of the nerve cells. By reflex time or reduced reflex time we may designate the time required for the processes in the center, — that is, the total time less that required for transmission of the impulse along the motor and sensory fibers and the latent period of the muscle con- tract ion. for the frog this is estimated as varying between 0.008 and 0.015 sec. In man the reflex time usually quoted is that given by Exner for the winking of the eye. He stimulated one lid electrically and recorded the reflex movement of the lid of the ol her eye. The total time for the reflex was, on an average, from 0.0578 sec. to 0.0662 ^-i'. Ih; estimated that the time for transmission of the impulse along the sensory and motor pal lis, together wit h the latent period of the muscle, amounted to 0.0107 sec So that the t rue reflex I ime from his determinations varied between 0.0 171 and 0.0555 sec. Mayhew,* using a more elaborate method, obtained for t he total I ime a mean figure equal to 0.0420 ^'C If Kxner's correction is applied then the true reflex time according to this de- termination is equal to 0.0313 see. In a series of experiments made upon frogs, in which the efferenl response to stimulation of the afferent fibers Of the sciatic nerve was measured by the electrical variation in the muscle involved, Buchanan finds that the delaj in the cord, when the reflex was on the same side, was * Mayhew, "Journal of Exp. Medicine," 2, 35, L897. REFLEX ACTIONS. 149 equal to 0.01 to 0.02 sec. If the reflex was on the crossed side about double this time was consumed in the cord. This delay of the velocity of transmission of an impulse in the nerve centers is a factor which must vary somewhat in different parts of the nervous system. It has been shown that in certain cases, at least, when strong stimuli are used the latent period of a reflex is not greater than would be accounted for by transmission through the nerve fibers and by the latency of the muscular contraction. Thus Francois Frank, in an experiment in which the gastrocnemius muscle of one side was made to contract reflexly by stimulation of the afferent root of a lumbar nerve on the other side, records a latent period of only 0.017 sec. Evidently in such a case there was no perceptible delay in passing through the nerve centers of the lumbar cord. Inhibition of Reflexes. — One of the most fundamental facts regarding spinal reflexes is the demonstration that they can be depressed or suppressed entirely — that is, inhibited — by other im- pulses reaching the same part of the spinal cord. The most sig- nificant experiment in this connection is that made by Setschenow.* If in a frog the entire brain or the cerebral hemispheres are re- moved, then stimulation of the exposed cut surface — for instance, by crystals of sodium chloricl — will depress greatly or perhaps inhibit entirely the usual spinal reflexes that may be obtained by cutaneous stimulation. On removal of the stimulating substance from the cut surface by washing with a stream of physiological saline (solution of sodium chlorid, 0.7 per cent.) the reflex activities of the cord are again exhibited in a normal way. This experiment accords with many facts which indicate that the brain may inhibit the activities of the spinal centers. In the reflex from tickling, for instance, we know that by a voluntary act we can repress the muscular movements up to a certain point; so also the limited control of the action of the centers of respiration and micturition is a phenomenon of the same character. To explain such acts we may assume the existence of a definite set of inhibitory fibers, arising in parts of the brain and distributed to the spinal cord, whose function is that of controlling the activities of the spinal centers. In view of the fact, however, that there is no independent proof of the existence of a separate set of inhibitor}' fibers within the central nervous system — that is, a set of fibers whose specific energy is that of inhibition — it is preferable to speak simply of the inhibitory influence of the brain upon the cord, leaving unde- cided the question as to whether this influence is exerted through a special set of fibers, or is brought about by some variation in * Setschenow, " Physiologische Studien iiber d. Hemmungs-Mechanisnien f. d. Reflexthatigkeit im Gehirn d. Frosches," Berlin, 1S63. 150 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. the time relations, intensity, or quality of the nerve impulses. Regarding the fact, however, there can be no question, and it constitutes a most important factor in the interaction of the dif- ferent parts of the nervous system. It is possible that this factor explains why a normal frog gives reflexes that are so much less constant and less predictable than one with its brain removed. A similar inhibition of spinal reflexes may be obtained by simul- taneous stimulation of two different parts of the skin. The usual reflex from pinching the toe of one leg may be inhibited in part or completely by simultaneous stimulation of the other leg or by direct electrical stimulation of an exposed nerve trunk. A similar interference is illustrated, perhaps, in the well-known device of inhibiting an act of sneezing by a strong sensory stimulation from some part of the skin — for instance, by pressing upon the upper lip. The importance of the process of inhibition in the normal movements of the body is illustrated strikingly by the phenomenon known as reciprocal inner rat ion, which has been investigated chiefly by Sherrington.* This observer has found that when a flexor muscle is stimulated reflexly there is at the same time a relaxation or loss of tone in its antagonistic extensor, which is explained as being due to an inhibition of the motor cells of the extensor in the cord. Reflex stimulation of the extensor is accompanied similarly by an inhibition of the tone of the antagonistic flexor. This phenomenon has been demonstrated not only for reflex stimulation of the cord but also for voluntary movements (Athanasieu) and for electrical stimulation of the cortical centers. The motor centers of the muscles surrounding the joints are apparently so connected in pairs that when one is excited the center of the corresponding antagonist is inhibited. This reciprocating mechanism dis- appear- under condil i < > 1 1 s , such as si rychnine-poisoning, in which, according i<> the usual belief, the irritability of the centers is greatly increased. A relationship quite comparable' to the reciprocal innervation, although working in only one direction, is exhibited by Hie peripheral nerve plexuses in the intestinal canal in the so-called law of the intestines (see p. 701). A brief statement of the more or less unsatisfactory theories of inhibit ion is given in connect ion with t he inhibitory action of the VagUS nerve "n the heart beat, (see p. 569). It- should be added, however, in lids Connection, that, stimulation of the cord, and probably of other parts of the nervous system, from two different sources may resull not only in an inhibition of the reflex normally occurring from one of the stimuli, but under some circumstances * Sherrington, "The Integrative Action of the Nervous System," 1906, p. 84. REFLEX ACTIONS. 151 may give an augmentation or reinforcement of the reflex. A striking example of this augmenting effect is given below in the paragraph upon the knee-kick. Influence of the Condition of the Cord on its Reflex Ac- tivities.— The time and extent of the reflex responses may be altered greatly by various influences, particularly by the action of drugs. The effect in such cases is usually upon the nerve centers, —that is, upon the cells themselves or upon the connections between the terminal arborization and the dendrites — the process of conduc- tion within the sensory and motor fibers being less easily affected. A convenient method of studying such influences is that employed by Turck. In this method the reflex frog is suspended, and the tip of the longest toe is immersed to a definite point in a solution of sulphuric acid of a strength of 0.1 to 0.2 per cent. If the time between the immersion and the reflex withdrawal of the foot is noted by a metronome, or by a record upon a kymograph, it will be found to be quite constant, provided the conditions are kept uniform. If the average time for this reflex is obtained from a series of observations it is possible to inject various substances — such as strychnin, chloroform, potassium bromid, quinin, etc.— under the skin, and after absorption has taken place to determine the effect by a new series of observations. So far as drugs are concerned the results of such experiments belong rather to pharma- cology than to physiology. The method in some cases brings out an interesting difference in the effects of various kinds of stimulation. Strychnin, for instance, as was stated above, increases greatly the delicacy of the reaction to pressure stimulation. At one stage in its action before the convulsive responses are obtained the threshold stimulus is greatly lowered, — mere contact with the toes causes a rapid retraction of the leg; whereas in the normal reflex frog a relatively large pressure is necessary to obtain a similar response. At this stage in the action of the strychnin the effect of the acid stimulus, on the contrary, may be markedly weakened so far as the time element is concerned. If the action of the strychnin is not too rapid, it is usually possible to find a point at which the time for the reflex is diminished, but this effect quickly disappears and the period between stimulus and response becomes markedly lengthened at a time when the slightest mechanical stimulation gives a rapid reflex movement. This paradoxical result may depend pos- sibly upon the variety of nerve fiber stimulated by the two kinds of stimuli or may be connected with the fact that the acid stimuli may bring about inhibitory as well as excitatory processes in the cord. Reflexes from Other Parts of the Nervous System. — Nu- merous typical reflexes are known to occur in the brain. The 152 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. reflex effects upon the important centers in the medulla, such as the vasomotor center, the respiratory center, and the cardio- inhibitory center, the winking of the eye, sneezing, the light reflex upon the sphincter muscle of the iris, and many other similar cases might be enumerated. All of these reactions will be described and discussed in their proper places. The conscious reactions of the brain are not included among the reflexes by virtue of the defi- nition which lays stress upon the involuntary characteristic of the reflex response, but it should be remembered that, so far as the nervous mechanism is concerned, these conscious reactions do not differ from the true reflexes. When we voluntarily move a limb the movement is guided and controlled by sensory impulses from the muscles put into action. The fibers of muscle sense from these muscles convey sensory impulses through a chain of neurons to the cortex of the brain and there the impulses doubtless affect and set into action the motor neurons through which the movement is effected. So far as we know, the discharges from the efferent neuron of the brain are not really automatic, but are conditioned or originated by stimuli from other neurons; so that the activities of the brain are carried on by a mechanism of one neuron acting on another, just as in the case of the reflex arc. The added feature of a psychical factor, a reaction in consciousness, enables us to draw a line of distinction between these activities and those of so-called pure reflexes; but the distinction is perhaps one of convenience only, for, although the extremes may be far enough apart to suit the definition, many intermediate instances may be found which an- difficult to classify. All skilled movements, for instance, such as walking, singing, dancing, bicycle riding, and the like, — although in the beginning obviously effected by voluntary co-ordination, nevertheless in the end, in proportion to the skill obtained, become more or less entirely reflex, — that is, involuntary. In learning such movements one must, as the saying goes, establish his reflexes, and the result can hardly be understood otherwise than by suppos- ing thai the continual adjustment of certain sensory impulses to certain co-ordinaled movements results in the formation of a more or less complex reflex arc, a eel of pal hs of least resistance. Reflexes through Peripheral Ganglia Axon Reflexes.— Many attempts have been made by physiologists to ascertain whether or not reflexes can occur through the peripheral nerve ganglia, lying outside the central nervous system. With regard to the posterior root ganglia, it has usually been supposed that they cannot exhiliit reflexes. When the posterior rool con- necting auch a ganglion to the cord is severed, then, according to our usual conception, the cells in the ganglia are cut off from all connections with the peripheral tissues by efferent REFLEX ACTIONS. 153 paths. This usual view may not, however, be correct. On the physiological side we have the fact (see p. 83) that stimu- lation of certain of the posterior root ganglia undei such cir- cumstances does give peripheral effects on the blood-vessels, causing a vascular dilatation in a certain region. On the histological side Cajal* and others have shown that some of these cells are provided with a pericellular nerve network, which is an afferent path so far as the cell is con- cerned, while the axon of the cell con- stitutes an efferent path. Whether these cells form a special group of efferent cells lying within the sensory ganglion, or whether they are sensory cells discharging into the cord and stimulated reflexly through the nerve network as well as through the peripheral process of the axon, cannot be said. The subject is one full of interest to physiology. In the ganglia of the sympathetic nerve and its appen- dages and in the similar ganglia contained in many of the organs the nerve cells have dendritic processes, and, so far as their histology is concerned, it would seem possi- ble that in any ganglion of this type there might be sensory and motor neurons so connected as to make the ganglion an independent reflex center. Numerous experiments have been made to determine experimentally whether reflexes can be obtained through such ganglia. Perhaps the most successful of these experiments have been made upon the inferior mesenteric ganglion. This ganglion may be isolated from all connections with the central nervous system and left attached to the bladder through the two hypogastric nerves (see Fig. 277). If now one of these nerves is cut and the central stump is stimulated, a contraction of the bladder follows. Obviously in this case the impulse has traveled to the ganglion and clown the other hy- pogastric nerve; the reaction has every appearance of being a true reflex. Nevertheless, Langley and Anderson, f who have studied the matter with especial care, are convinced that in this * Cajal, "Ergebnisse der Anat. u. Entwickelungsgeschichte, " vol. xvi., 1906. t Langley and Anderson, "Journal of Physiology," 16, 410, 1894. Fig. 69.— Schema to show idea of an axon re- flex: The preganglionic fiber, a, sends branches to two postganglionic fibers, b, c. If stimulated at x the impulse passes backward in a direction the reverse of normal and falling into b and c gives a pseudoreflex effect. 1")4 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. and similar case.-? we have to do with what they call pseudo- reflexes or axon reflexes. The idea underlying this term may be explained in this way: Every sympathetic ganglion is connected with the central- nervous system, brain and cord, by efferent spinal fibers, preganglionic fibers, which terminate by arborization around the dendrites of the sympathetic cells. The efferent fibers arising from the latter may be designated as post- ganglionic fibers. These authors give reasons to believe that any one preganglionic fiber, a, Fig. 69, may connect by collaterals with several sympathetic cells. If such a fiber were stimulated at x, then the impulse passing back along the axon in a direction the reverse of normal would stimulate cells b and c, giving effects that are apparently reflex, but which differ from true reflexes in that the stimulating axon belongs to a motor neuron. Under normal circumstances it is not probable that an effect of this kind can be produced. The Tonic Activity of the Spinal Cord. — In addition to the definite reflex activities of the cord, each traceable to a distinct sensory stimulus, there is evidence to show that many of its motor neurons are in that state of more or less continuous activity which we designate as tonic activity or tonus. There is abundant reason for this belief in regard to many of the special centers of the cord and brain, such as the vasomotor center, the center for the sphinc- ter muscle of the iris, the centers for the sphincter muscles of the bladder, the anus, etc. But the evidence includes the motor neurons to the voluntary as well as the involuntary musculature. In a decapitated frog the muscles take a definite position, and Brondgeest showed that if such an animal is suspended, after cut- ting the sciatic plexus in one leg, the leg on the uninjured side takes a more flexed position. The explanation offered for this result is thai the muscles on the sound side are being innervated by the motor neurons of the cord. Inasmuch as a result of this kind cannot be obtained from a frog whose skin has been removed, Or in one in which the posterior roots have been severed it seems evident that this tonic discharge from the motor neurons is due to a constant inflow of impulses along the sensory paths. The muscle tonus, in other words, is really :i reflex tonus, which differs from ordinary reflex movements only in the absence of a sudden, visible contraction and in the more or less continuous character of the innervation. In the section on animal heat, the importance of this constant innervation of the muscles as a source of heat \& further emphasized. The idea of a more or less continuous but vai ■■. ing activity of the centers in the brain and cord in consequence of the continuous inflow of impulses along the sensory paths fits in very well with many facts observed in the peripheral organs, — REFLEX ACTIONS. 155 facts that will be referred to from time to time as the physiology of these organs is considered. Effects of Removal of the Spinal Cord. — Numerous investi- gators have sectioned the cord partly or completely at various levels. The general results of these experiments as regards loss of sensation or voluntary movement are described in the next section treating of the cord as a path of conduction to and from the brain. But attention may be called here to some of the gen- eral results obtained by Goltz* in some remarkable experiments in which the entire cord was removed with the exception of the cervical region and a small portion of the upper thoracic. In making this experiment it was necessary to perform the operation in several steps. That is, the cord was first sectioned in the upper thoracic region and then in successive operations the lower tho- racic, lumbar, and sacral regions were removed completely. Very great care was necessary in the treatment of the animals after these operations, but some survived and lived for long periods, the digestive, circulatory, and excretory organs performing their functions in a normal manner. The muscles of the hind limbs and trunk, however, underwent complete atrophy, owing to the destruction of their motor nerves. The blood-vessels also were paralyzed after the first operations, but gradually their muscu- lature again recovered tone, showing that, although under normal conditions the tonic contraction of the vessels is under the in- fluence of nerves arising from the cord, this tone may be re-estab- lished in time after the severance of all spinal connections. Some of the specific results of these experiments, bearing upon the re- flexes of defecation, micturition, and parturition, will be described later. Attention may be called here to the general results illustrating the general functions of the cord. In the first place, there was, of course, a total paralysis of volun- tary movement in the muscles innervated normally through the parts of the cord removed, and a complete loss of sensation in the same regions, particularly of cutaneous and muscular sensibility. In the second place, the visceral organs, including the blood-vessels, were shown to be much more independent of the direct control of the central nervous system. While these organs in the experiments under consideration were still in connection with the sympathetic ganglia and in part with the brain through the vagi, still their connections with the central nervous system, particularly as regards their sensory paths and the innervation of the blood-vessels, were in largest part destroyed. The immediate effect of this destruction would have been the death of the animal if the care * Goltz and Ewald, "Pfl tiger's Archiv fur die gesarnmte Physiologic, " 63, 362, 1896. 156 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. of the observer hud not replaced, in the beginning, the normal control exercised by the nervous system through the spinal nerves; but later this careful nursing was not required. While these organs, therefore, are capable of a certain amount of independent activity and co-ordination, they are normally controlled through the various reflex activities of the brain and cord. In the third place, it is noteworthy that the adaptability of the cordless portion of the animal was distinctly less than normal. Its power of preserving a constant body temperature was more limited than in the normal animal, and the susceptibility to inflammatory disturbances in the visceral organs was greatly increased. It seems evident, from these facts, that, although the animal was living, its power of adaptation to marked changes in the external or internal environment was greatly lessened, and this fact illustrates well the great general importance of the spinal cord and brain as reflex centers controlling the nutri- tion and co-ordinated activities of the body tissues and organs. This control is necessary under normal conditions for the success- ful combination of the activities of the various organs. A large part of this control is doubtless dependent upon the regulation of the blood supply to the various organs. The mechanism by which this is effected and the parts played by the cord and the brain (medulla oblongata), respectively, will be described in the section on ( 'irculat ion. Knee-jerk. —Knee-jerk or knee-kick is the name commonly given to the jerk of the foot when a light blow is struck upon the patellar ligament just below the knee. The jerk of the foot is due to a contraction of the quadriceps femoris muscle. Accord- ing to Sherrington, the parts of this muscular mass chiefly concerned are the m. vastus medialis and m. vastus intermedins. In order to obtain the muscular response it is usually neces- sary to put the quadriceps under some ten-ion by flexion of the leg. This end is achieved most readily by crossing the knees or by allowing the leg to hang freely when sitting on the edge of a bench or table. Under such circumstances the jerk is obtained in the greal majority of normal persons, and this fad has made it an im port ant diagnostic sign in many diseases of the spinal cord. The import mice of 1 he reaction for such purposes was first brought out by the work of Erb and Westphal* in 1875. Reinforcement of the Knee-jerk. It was first shown by Jendrassik (1883) thai the extent of the jerk may be greatly aug- mented if, at the time i he blow is si ruck upon the tendon, a si rong voluntary movement is made by the individual, such as squeezing the hands together tightly or clenching the jaws. This phenomenon * Erb and We tphal, "Archiv f. Psychiatrie," L875, vol. v. REFLEX ACTIONS. 157 was studied carefully in this country by Mitchell and Lewis,* who ascertained that a similar augmentation may be produced by giving the individual a simultaneous sensory stimulation. They desig- nated the phenomenon as a reinforcement, and this name is gen- erally employed by English writers, although occasionally the term "Bahnung," introduced byExner to describe a similar phenom- enon, is also used. It is found that by a reinforcement the knee- jerk may be demonstrated in some individuals in whom the ordi- nary blow upon the tendon fails to elicit a response. Bowditch and Warrenf studied the phenomenon of reinforcement and brought out a fact of very great interest. They studied especially the time interval between the blow upon the tendon and the reinforcing act and found that if the latter preceded the blow by too great an inter- val then, instead of an augmentation of the jerk, there was a dimi- nution which they designated as negative reinforcement or inhi- bition. This inhibiting effect began to appear when the reinforcing act (hand-squeeze) preceded the blow by an interval of from 0.22 to 0.6 sec, and the maximum inhibiting effect was obtained at an Fig. 70. — Showing in millimeters the amount by which the "reinforced" knee-kick varied from the normal, the level of which is represented by the horizontal line at 0, "nor- mal." The time intervals elapsing between the clenching of the hands (which constituted the reinforcement) and the tap on the tendon are marked below. The reinforcement is greatest when the two events are nearly simultaneous. At an interval of 0.4 sec. it amounts to nothing; during the next 0.6 sec. the height of the kick is actually diminished, while after an interval of 1 sec. the negative reinforcement tends to disappear; and when 1.7 sec. is allowed to elapse the height of the kick ceases to be affected by the clenching of the hands. — (Bowditch and Warren.) interval of from 0.6 to 0.9 sec. Beyond this point the effect became less noticeable, and at an interval of 1.7 to 2.5 sec. the reinforcing act had no influence at all upon the jerk. These relations are shown in the accompanying curve (Fig. 70). These authors con- * Mitchell and Lewis, " American Journal of Med. Sciences," 92, 363, 1886. f Bowditch and Warren, "Journal of Physiology," 2, 25, 1890. 158 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. firmed also i ho fact that a sensory stimulus, such as a gentle blast of air on the conjunctiva or the knee, may reinforce the jerk. The physiological explanation of the reinforcement, negative and posi- tive, is a matter of inference only, but the view usually held is that it is due to "overflow." That is, many facts, such as strychnin tetanus, indicate that the neuromuscular machinery of the entire central nervous system is more or less directly connected and that functional activity at one part may influence the irritability of the remainder, either in the direction of reinforcement (Bahnung) or inhibition. We may conceive, therefore, that when the hands are squeezed the motor impulses sent down from the cortex of the brain to the upper portion of the cord overflow to some extent, sufficient at least to alter the irritability of the other motor neurons in the cord. Experimental stimulation of the cortex has given similar results. Exner* found that when the motor center for the foot in the cortex of a rabbit was stimulated, the stimulation, even if too weak to be effective itself, caused an increase in the contraction brought about refiexly by a simultaneous stimulation of the skin of the paw, and furthermore if these stimuli were so reduced in strength that each was ineffective, then when applied together a contraction was obtained. In this case an ineffective stimulus from the cortex reaching the spinal cord increased the irritability of the motor centers there so that a simultaneous reilex stimulus from the foot, ineffective in itself, became effective. Is the Knee-jerk a Reflex? — The most interesting question in this connection is whether the jerk is a true reilex act or is due to a direct mechanical stimulation of the muscle. Opinions are divided upon this point. Those who believe that the jerk is a reflex lay emphasis upon the undoubted fact that the; integrity of the reflex arc is absolutely essential to the response. The quad- riceps receives its motor and sensory fibers through the femoral nerve, and pathological lesions upon man as well as direct experimental investigation upon monkeys prove thai if either the posterior or anterior roots of the third mid fourth lumbar spinal nerve- are destroyed the knee-jerk disappears entirely. The oppo- nents of the redex view explain this fact by the theory that in order tor the quadriceps to respond it, must be in a condition oi tonus. This tonus depends upon the reflex arc, the sensory impulses from l he muscle Serving to keep it in that condition of subdued contraction known as tone. On this view destruc- tion of the reflex arc renden the muscle less irritable, so that if, will not respond by a contraction to the sudden mechanical exten- sion or pull caused by the blow on the temlon. The adherents of thi- view lay emphasis upon two facts: First, the knee-jerk is a * Exner, " Vrchiv f. 'Ii<- gesammte Phy iologie," 27, 112, 1882. REFLEX ACTIONS. 159 simple contraction, and not a tetanus, and, generally speaking, the motor centers of the cord discharge a series of impulses when stimulated. In answer to this objection it may be said that while muscular contractions produced reflexly are usually tetanic, it does not follow that this is invariably the case. Sher- rington* has shown, for instance, that an undoubted reflex designated by him as the "extensor thrust," which also involves the extensor muscles of the hind leg, is very short lasting, requir- ing perhaps only A- sec, and judged by this standard is as much of a simple contraction as the knee-jerk. The "extensor thrust" is a sharp contraction of the extensor muscles of the hind leg aroused by pressure upon the plantar surface of the hind foot. Second, the time for the jerk — that is, the interval between the stimulus and the response — is too short for a reflex. The determination of this time has been attempted by many ob- servers for the purpose of deciding the controversy, but unfor- tunately the results have been lacking in uniformity, varying, in man, from 0.025 to 0.073 sec. Applegarth, making use of a dog with a severed spinal cord, obtained for the time of the knee-jerk an interval of 0.014 to 0.02 sec; Waller and Gotch, using the rabbit, found the time to be only 0.008 to 0.005 sec Other figures would appear to indicate that the latent period is shorter the smaller the animal, a fact which in itself would imply that some factor other than the latency of the muscle itself enters into the time required. And if we accept the newer figures in regard to the velocity of the nerve impulse in mammalian nerves at the body temperature (see p. 113) there would seem to be sufficient time in all cases for the impulse to get to the cord and back. Thus, in the dog, if we take the velocity of the impulse as 120,000 mm. per second, and the distance to be travelled as 800 mm., only 0.0066 sec. would be necessary to make the circuit, assuming that there is no appreciable delay in the centers. In view of these figures it seems quite unjustifiable to conclude that the knee-jerk is not a reflex simply because the time between the stimulus and the response is shorter than with some other known reflexes. It will be remembered in this connection that one observer (p. 149) records a genuine reflex time for this region as short as 0.017 sec. So also the fact that under certain conditions a genuine crossed knee-jerk may be obtained, that is, an extension of the other leg, speaks in favor of the view that the usual jerk is a reflex phenomenon. Conditions Influencing the Extent of the Knee-jerk.: — The effect of various normal conditions upon the knee-jerk has been * Sherrington, "The Integrative Action of the Nervous System," 1906. 160 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. studied by a number of observers, particularly by Lombard.* The results are most interesting in that they indicate very clearly that the irritability of the spinal cord varies with almost every marked change in mental activity. During sleep the jerk disappears and in mental conditions of a restful character its extent is relatively small. In conditions of mental excitement or irritation, on the contrary, the jerk becomes markedly increased. Lombard ob- served also, in his own case, a daily rhythm, which is represented in the chart given in Fig. 71. It would seem from his experiments Fig. 71. — Lombard'8 ficure to indicate the daily rhythm in the extent of the knee- t'erk and the effect of mental stimuli. The ordinate.s (0-110) represent the extent of the :iek in millimeters. Bach dot represents a .separate kick, while the heavy horizontal line gives the average extent for the period indicated. that the extent of the knee-jerk is a sensitive indicator of the relative state of irrit ability of the nervous system: "The knee- jerk ifi increased and diminished by whatever increases and di- mini In- the activity of the central nervous system as a whole." Tlii- general fact is supported, especially as regards mental activity, by observations Oil other similar mechanisms, --such, for instance, as the condition of the nervous centers controlling the bladder. Use of the Knee-jerk and Spinal Reflexes as Diagnostic Signs. — Tin- fact thai the knee-jerk depends on the integrity of the reflex arc in the lumbar cord has made it useful as a diagnostic indication in Lesions of the cord, particularly, of course, For the ♦Lombard, "The American Journal <>f Psychology," 1887, |>. I, Set; ;il " article "Knee-jerk" (Warren), "Wood'a Etef. Handbook of Med. Sci- ," econd edition, L902. REFLEX ACTIONS. 161 lumbar region. It is mainly on account of its practical value and the ease with which it is ordinarily obtained that the phenom- enon has been studied so extensively. In the disease known as progressive locomotor ataxia the posterior root fibers in the pos- terior columns in the lumbar region are affected, and, as a con- sequence, the jerk is diminished or abolished altogether according to the stage of the disease. So also lesions affecting the anterior horns of the gray matter will destroy the reflex b3^ cutting off the motor path, while in other cases lesions in the brain or the lateral columns of the cord affecting the pyramidal system of fibers may be accompanied by an exaggeration of this and similar reflexes. This latter fact agrees with the experimental results (see p. 149) upon ablation of the brain. After such operations in the frog and lower mammals at least the spinal reflexes may show a marked increase. Interruption of the descending connections between brain and cord at any point, therefore, may be accompanied by a strik- ing increase in sensitiveness of the spinal reflexes. The explana- tion usually given is that the inhibitory influences of the brain centers upon the cord are thereby weakened or destroyed. The explanation is incomplete in that it leaves undecided the question as to whether this inhibitory influence is exerted through the ordinary motor paths or through a special set of inhibitory fibers. Other Spinal Reflexes. — Various other distinctive reflexes through the spinal cord may be obtained readily, and since the motor cells concerned lie at different levels in the cord the presence, absence, or modified character of these reflexes has been used frequently for diagnostic purposes. In the first place there are a number of so-called deep reflexes which may be aroused by sensory stimulation of parts beneath the skin, such as the tendons, ligaments, and periosteum. Almost any tendon if stimulated mechanically may give a jerk of the cor- responding muscle, just as in the case of the knee-kick. Such reactions have been described and used in the case of the wrist- jerk, the jaw-jerk, the Achilles-jerk, etc. The last named is obtained by putting the foot into a position of dorsiflexion and then tapping the tendo calcaneus (Achillis). The result is a contraction of the gastrocnemius, causing plantar flexion of the foot. A variation of this reflex is the phenomenon known as ankle clonus. This is obtained by giving a quick forcible dorsiflexion to the foot thus putting the tendon and muscle under a sudden mechanical strain. In some cases there results a rhythmical series of contractions of the gastrocnemius. A second group of reflexes may be obtained by stimulation of special points on the skin, the cutaneous reflexes. For example, the plantar reflex, which consists in a flexion of the toes when the 11 162 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. sole of the foot is stimulated by tactile or painful stimuli. Under pathological conditions which involve a lesion of the pyramidal tracts in the cord this reflex is altered, the great toe being extern lei 1 instead of flexed (Babinski's phenomenon). The cremasteric reflex consists in a contraction of the cremasteric muscle which raises the testis. It follows from stimulation of the skin on the inner side of the thigh at the level of the scrotum. The location of the motor centers of these and other similar reflexes is shown in the accompanying illustration (Fig. 72). lit:. , _'. — Diagrammatic representation of the lower portion of the human buTb and spinal cord. The cord is divided into its four regions: 1, Medulla cerviealis; 2, medulla dorsalis; 3, medulla lumbalis; 4, medulla sacralis. Within each region the spinal segments bear Roman numbers. On the left side of the diagram the locality supplied by the sensory (afferent) neurons is indicated by one or more words, and these latter are connected with the bulb or the segments of the cord at the levels at which the nerves enter. The afferent character is indicated by the arrow tip on the lines of reference. On the right-hand side the names of muscles or groups of muscles are given, and to thern are drawn reference lines which start from the segments of the cord in which the cell-bodies of origin have been located. Within the cord itself, the designations for several reflex centers are inscribed in the segment where the mechanism is localized. For example, Reflexus scapularis, Centrum ciCo-epinale. Reflexus epigastricus, Reflexus abdominalis, Reflexus cremastericus, Reflexus patellar)*, Heflexus tendo Achillas. Centrum vesicale, Centrum anale (the last two on the left side of the diagram). (Donaldson, "Amer. Text-book of Physiology," from "looses Neurologic®," StriimpeU and Jakob.) Pharynx Oesophagus Larynx, Trachea Infestina {Thorax. Abdomen) Regio occipitalis' Regio colli Regio nuchae Regio humeri Regio Nervi radialis Regio N. meoiani N. Mm. pharyngis. palati Mm. iai yogis Mm. linguae Oesophagus Sternocleidomastoideus let i Diaphragma Supra-, Deltoi Biceps Pectoralis major (portio ciavicui.) Teres minor Extensores carpi et digitor. longi ) §= iorlportioi §:. Flexores carpi et digitor. longi If- lleopsoas . Sartorius Quadriceps femoris Biceps Semitendi— Semimembranosus Extensores digitorum Peronei Flexores digitorum Fig. 72. CHAPTER VIII. THE SPINAL CORD AS A PATH OF CONDUCTION. In addition to the varied and important functions performed by the cord as a system of reflex centers controlling the activities of numerous glands and visceral organs as well as the so-called voluntary muscles, it is physiologically most important as a path- way to and from the brain. All the fibers, numbering more than half a million, that enter the cord through the posterior roots of the spinal nerves bring in afferent impulses, which may be continued upward by definite tracts that end eventually in the cortex of the cerebrum, the cerebellum, or some other portion of the brain. On the other hand, many of the efferent impulses originating reflexly or otherwise in different parts of the brain are conducted downward into the cord to emerge at one or another of the anterior roots of the spinal nerves. The location and extent of these ascending and descending paths form a part of the inner structure of the cord, which is most important practically in medical diagnosis and which has been the subject of a vast amount of experimental inquiry in physiology, anatomy, pathology, and clinical medicine. In working out this inner architecture the neuron conception has been of the greatest value, and the results are usually presented in terms of these interconnecting units. The Arrangement and Classification of the Nerve Cells in the Gray Matter of the Cord. — Nerve cells are scattered throughout the gray matter of the cord, but are arranged more or less distinctly in groups or, considering the longitudinal aspect of the cord, in columns the character of which varies somewhat in the different regions. From the standpoint of physiological anatomy these cells may be grouped into four classes: (1) The anterior root cells, clustered in the anterior column of gray matter (1, Fig. 73). The axons of these cells pass out of the cord almost at once to form the anterior or motor roots of the spinal nerves. (2) The tract cells, so called because their axons instead of leaving the cord by the spinal roots enter the white matter and, after passing upward or downward, help to form the tracts into which this white matter may be divided (2 and 3 of Fig. 73) . These tract cells are found throughout the gray matter, and according to the side on which the axon enters into a tract they may be divided into three subgroups: 163 164 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. (a) Those whose axons enter the white matter on the same side of the cord, the tautomeric tract cells of Van Gehuchten. (b) Those whose axons pass through the anterior white commissure and thus reach the tracts in the white matter of the other side. These arc known as commissural cells or the heteromeric tract coll- of Van ( rehuchten. They form one obvious means for crossed conduction in the cord, (c) Those whose axons divide into two, one j:>assing into the white matter of the same side, the other pass- ing by way of the anterior commissure to reach the white matter of the opposite side — the hecateromeric tract cells of Van Gehuch- ten. (3) The Golgi cells of the second type — that is, cells whose Uentral Fjk. 73. — Schema of the structure of the cord. — After Lenhossek.) On the right the nerve cells; on tin- left the entering nerve fibers. Right side: 1, Motor cells, anterior column, giving rise to the fibers "f the anterior root; '-', tract cells whose axons pass into the white matter of I hi' an t et ior and lateral funiculi; '-, commissural eel Is whose axons pass chiefly through the anterior commissure to reach the anterior funiculi of the other side; 4, Golgi cells i econd I pe), whose axon do nol leave the may matter; .r>, tract cells whose ;.i into the white matter of the posterior funiculi. Leftside: L, Entering libers of tin- po terioi io.it, ending, from within outward, as follows: Clarke's column, posterior Column ol oppo ite id'-, anterior Column -a me side I re Ilex arc), lateral coin i mi of same side, posterioi column of am.- ide; 'J. collateral-, from fibers in the anterior and lateral funiculi, ■';, collateral ol descending pyramidal fibei ending around motor cells in anterior column. axons divide into a number of small branches like those of a dendrite. The axons of those cells, therefore, do not become medullated nerve fibers; they take no part in the formation of pinal roots or the tracts of white matter in the cord, but terminate diffusely within the gray mailer itself. (I) Tin; pos- terior root cells lying toward the base of I he anterior coin inns. These cells have l><-<-n demonstrated in some of the lower verte- brates (petromyzon- chick embryo), but I heir existence in the mammal i- -till a question in some doubt; their axons pass out from t he cord by t he posterior root and t hoy form t he anatomical evidence for the vie^ thai the posterior roots may contain souk; SPINAL CORD AS A PATH OF CONDUCTION. 165 efferent fibers. Some of the groups of tract cells have been given special names — such, for instance, as the dorsal nucleus (Clarke's column). This group of cells lies at the inner angle of the posterior column of gray matter (5, Fig. 76), and forms a column usually described as extending from the middle lumbar to the upper dorsal region. The axons from these cells pass to the dorsal margin of the lateral funiculi on the same side to con- stitute an ascending tract of fibers known as the tract of Flechsig, or the fasciculus cerebellospinalis. General Relations of the Gray and White Matter in the Cord. — Cross-sections of the cord at different levels show that the relative amounts of gray and white matter differ considerably at different levels, so that it is quite possible to recognize easily from what region any given section is taken. At the cervical and the lumbar enlargements the amounts of both gray and white matter — that is, the total cross-area of the cord — show a sudden White matter. Gray matter. —Entire section. 100 80 eo ^_^- — ^^xComposite curves based an 4 Cases. • .. ~^. ■ ---- — 40 so 100 Juinoryvinismi n m rc y va yn ym k x xi xu i n jukyi nuiim Fig. 74. — Curves to show the relative areas of the gray and white matter of the spinal cord at different levels. — {Donaldson and Davis.) The Roman numerals along the abscissa represent the origin of the different spinal nerves. increase owing to the larger number of fibers arising at these levels. The white matter, and therefore the total cross-area, shows also a constant increase from below upward, due to the fact that in the upper regions many fibers exist that have come into the cord at a lower level or from the brain, those from the latter region being gradually distributed to the spinal nerves as we proceed downward. In the accompanying figure a curve is presented showing the cross- area of the cord and the relative amounts of gray and white matter at each segment. Tracts in the White Matter of the Cord, Methods of Deter- mining.— -The separation of the medullated fibers of the cord into distinct tracts of fibers possessing different functions has been accomplished in part by the combined results of investiga- tions in anatomy, physiology, and pathology. The two methods that have been employed most frequently and to the best advan- tage are the method of secondary degeneration (Wallerian degen- 106 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. eration) and the method of myelinization. The method of second- ary degeneration depends upon the fact that, when a fiber is cut off from its cell of origin, the peripheral end degenerates in a few days. If, therefore, a lesion, experimental or pathological, is made in the cord at any level, those fibers that are affected undergo degeneration: those with their cells below the lesion degenerate up- ward, and those with their cells above the lesion degenerate down- ward. According to the law of polarity of conduction in the neuron a descending degeneration in the cord indicates motor or efferent paths as regards the brain, and ascending degeneration indicates sensory or afferent paths. It is obvious that localized lesions can be used in this way to trace definite groups of fibers through the cord. If, for instance, one exposes and cuts the posterior roots in one or more of the lumbar nerves, the portions of the fibers entering the cord will degenerate, and the path of some of these fibers may be traced in this way upward to the medulla. The degenerated fibers may be revealed histologically by the staining methods of Weigert or of Marchi. The latter method (preservation in Midler's fluid, staining in osmic acid and Muller's fluid) has proved to be espe- cially useful; the degenerated fibers during a certain period give a black color with this liquid, owing probably to the splitting up of the lecithin in the myelin and the liberation of the fat from its combination with the other portions of the molecule.* The mye- linization method was introduced by Flechsig. It depends upon the fact that in the embryo the nerve fibers as first formed have no myelin sheath, and that this easily detected structure is in the central nervous system assumed at about the same time by those bundles or tracts of fibers that have a common course and func- tion. By this means the origin and termination of certain tracts may be worked out in the embryo or shortly after birth. The well-known system of pyramidal fibers, for instance, is clearly differentiated in the embryo late in intra-uterine life or at birth, owing to the fact that the fibers composing it have not at that time acquired their myelin sheaths. Flechsig assumes that the development of the myelin marks the completed structure of the nerve fiber and indicates, therefore, the time of its entrance into full fund tonal activity. General Classification of the Tracts. — The tracts that have been worked out in the white matter of the cord have been classified in everal ways. We have, in the first, place, the division into as- cending and descending tracts. This division rests upon the fact that the axon conducts its impulses away from the cell of origin, and consequently I hose neurons whose axons extend upward toward the * Sec Halliburton, "The Chemical Side of Nervous Activity," London, 1901; "Croonian Lectures." SPINAL CORD AS A PATH OP CONDUCTION. 167 higher parts of the cord or brain are designated as ascending, since normally the impulses conducted by them take this direction. They constitute the afferent or sensory paths, and in case of injury to the fiber or cell the secondary degeneration also extends upward. The reverse, of course, holds true for the descending or motor paths. The tracts may be divided also into long and short (or segmental) tracts. The latter group comprises those tracts or fibers which have only a short course in the white matter, extending over a dis- tance of one or more spinal segments. Histologically the fibers of these tracts take their origin from the tract cells in the gray matter of the cord and after running in the white matter for a distance of one or more segments they again enter the gray matter to terminate around the dendritic processes of another neuron. These short tracts may be ascending or descending, and the impulses that they conduct are conveyed up or down the cord by a series of neurons, each of whose axons runs only a short distance in the white matter, and then conveys its impulse to another neuron whose axon in turn extends for a segment or two in the white matter, and so on. These tracts are sometimes described as association or short associa- tion tracts, because they form the mechanism by which the activi- ties of different segments of the cord are brought into association. This method of conduction by segmental relays involving the par- ticipation of a series of neurons may be regarded as the primitive method. It indicates the original structure of the cord as a series of segments, each more or less independent physiologically. The short tracts in the mammalian cord he close to the gray matter, forming the bulk of what is known as the anterior and lateral proper fasciculi. The long tracts, on the contrary, are com- posed of those fibers, ascending or descending, which run a long distance, and, in fact, extend from the cord to some part of the brain. It is known, however, that, although the tracts as tracts extend from brain to cord, many of their constituent fibers may begin and end in the cord or in the brain, as the case may be. Some of the fibers of the long tracts are, there- fore, so far as the cord is concerned, simply long association tracts which connect different regions — e. g., cervical and lum- bar— of the cord by a single neuron, as the short asso- ciation tracts connect different segments of the same region. It is said that in these long tracts those fibers that have the shortest course lie to the inside — that is, nearest to the gray matter.* From the results of comparative studies of the different vertebrates we may conclude that the long tracts are a relatively late development in their phylogenetic history, and that in the most highly developed animals, man and the anthropoid apes, * Sherrington and Laslett, "Journal of Physiology," 29, 188, 1903; and Sherrington, ibid., 14, 255. 16S PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. these long tracts are more conspicuous and form a larger per- centage of the total area of the cord. A physiological corollary of this conclusion should he that in man the independent activity of the cord is less marked than in the lower vertebrates, and this deduction is borne out by facts (see p. 147). Specific Designation of the Long Spinal Tracts. — The tracts that are most satisfactorily determined for the human spinal cord are indicated schematically in Fig. 75. They are named as follows: In the posterior funiculus, 1. The fasciculus gracilis (column of Goll), 2. The fasciculus cuneatus (column of Burdach). ventral pd 0 / *> g lit'. 75, Schema of the tracts in the spinal cord (Kolliker) : n. Fasciculus grapihs; /,. fasciculus cuneatu ; pc, fa ciculus cerebrospinalis lateralis; pd, fasciculus cerebrospinalis anterioi ; /. fasciculus cerebellospinalis ; gr, fasciculus anterolateral superficialis. In t lie Lateral funiculus, 1. The fasciculus cerebrospinalis lateralis, known also as the lateral or crossed pyramidal I ract. 2. The fasciculus cerebellospinalis, known also as Flechsig's i pad . :;. The fasciculus anterolateral superficialis, known also as Gower's trad . 1. The lateral ground bundle (fasciculus lateralis proprius), made up chiefly of short association fibers. In t he anterior funiculus, 1. The fasciculus cerebrospinalis anterior, known also as the direct or anterior pyramidal I rad . 2. The anterior ground bundle (fasciculus anterior proprius). SPINAL CORD AS A PATH OF CONDUCTION. 169 Of these tracts, the fasciculus gracilis, fasciculus cuneatus, fasciculus cerebellospinalis, and fasciculus anterolateralis super- ficialis represent ascending or sensory paths, while the lateral and anterior cerebrospinal or pyramidal fasciculi form a related descending or motor path. It will be convenient to describe first the connections and physiological significance of these tracts and then refer briefly to the other less definitely estab- lished ascending and descending paths. The Termination in the Cord of the Fibers of the Posterior Root. — All sensory fibers from the limbs and trunk enter the cord through the posterior roots. Inasmuch as these roots are superfi- cially connected with the posterior funiculi, the older observers naturally supposed that this portion of the white matter of the cord forms the pathway for sensory impulses passing to the brain. That this sup- position is not entirely cor- rect was proved by experi- mental physiology. Sec- tion of the posterior fu- niculi causes little or no obvious loss of sensations in the parts below the lesion. Histological inves- tigation has since shown that only a portion of the fibers entering through the posterior root continue up the cord in the posterior funiculi; some and indeed a large proportion of the whole number enter into the gray matter and end around tract cells, whence the path is continued upward by the axons of these latter cells, mainly in the lateral or anterolateral funiculi. The several ways in which the posterior root fibers may end in the cord are indicated in Fig. 76. The posterior roots contain fibers of different diameters, and those of smallest size (1) are found collected into an area known Fig. 76. — Schema to show the terminations of the entering fibers of the posterior root : 1, Fibers entering zone of Lissauer and terminating in posterior column; 2, fiber terminating around a tract cell whose axon passes into white matter of same side; 3, fiber terminating around a tract cell whose axon passes to opposite side (commissural cell); 4, fiber terminating around motor cell of anterior column (reflex arc); 5, fiber terminating in tract cell of dorsal nucleus; 6, fiber (exog- enous) passing upward in posterior funiculus to terminate in the medulla oblongata. 1(0 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. as the zone of Lissauer, lying between the periphery of the cord and the tip of the posterior column. These fibers enter the gray matter chiefly in the posterior column of the same side and end around tract cells. The larger fibers of the root lying to the median side fall into two groups: Those lying laterally (2, 3, 4) enter the gray matter of the posterior column and end in tract cells (2) whose axons are distributed to the same side of the cord, or in tract cells whose axons (3) pass to the other side through the anterior white commissure, or in the motor cells of the anterior column, thus making a typical reflex arc. Some of the fibers of this group may also pass through the posterior commissure, to end in the gray matter of the opposite side. The larger fibers lying nearest to the median line enter the fas- ciculus cuneatus and run forward in the cord, some of them (6) continuing upward to the medulla, and some of them (5), after a shorter course, turning into the gray matter to end in the cells of the dorsal nucleus. The axons of the cells in the dorsal nucleus in turn pass out of the gray matter to constitute the ascending path in the lateral funiculus, known as the cerebello- spinal fasciculus. This general outline of the mode of ending in the cord of the fibers of the posterior root is complicated further by the fact that these fibers are supposed to give off collaterals after entering the cord. The course of the typical filter in the posterior root is represented in Fig. 67. According to this diagram, the root fiber, after entering the cord, makes a Y or T division, one branch passing downward or posteriorly for a short distance, the other, longer division, passing upward or anteriorly. Each of these main stems may give off one or more lateral branches, sensory collaterals. A main stem, therefore, which runs upward in the fasciculus cuneatus (6) to terminate in the medulla oblongata may give off collaterals at various levels which terminate in the gray matter of the cord, either around tract cells or around the anterior root cells, forming in the latter case a simple reflex arc. The existence of collaterals upon the root fibers within the cord has been demonstrated in the human embryo, but we have little exact information concerning their numerical value in the adult. The schema given in Fig. 7(i must, therefore, be accepted as an entirely diagrammatic representation of the chief possibilities of the mode of ending of the fibers of the posterior roof by way of their collaterals as well as by way of the main stems. Ascending (Afferent or Sensory) Paths in the Posterior Funiculi. The posterior funiculi are composed partly of fibers derived directly from t he posterior roots (6 in schema) and arising, therefore, from the cells in the posterior root ganglia, and partly SPINAL CORD AS A PATH OF CONDUCTION. 171 from fibers that arise from tract cells in cord itself. It is convenient to speak of the former group as exogenous fibers, using this term to designate nerve fibers which arise from cells placed outside the cord; and the latter group as endo- genous fibers — that is, fibers that have their cells of origin in the gray matter of the cord. If we omit a consideration of their collaterals the course of the exogenous fibers is easily understood. They come into the cord at every pos- terior root, enter into the fasciculus cuneatus, and pass upward. The fibers of this kind that enter at the lower regions, sacral and lumbar, are, however, gradually pushed toward the median line by the exogenous fibers entering at higher levels, so that in the upper tho- racic or cervical regions the fasciculus gracilis is composed mainly of exogenous fibers that have entered the cord in the lumbar or sacral region. These fibers continue upward to end in two groups of cells that lie on the dorsal side of the medulla oblongata, and are known, respectively, as the nucleus of the funiculus gracilis (or nucleus of Goll) and the nucleus o'f the funiculus cunea- tus (or nucleus of Burdach). Their path forward from the medulla is con- tinued by new neurons arising in these nuclei, and will be described later. The course of these fibers in the cord may be shown beautifully by the method of secondary degeneration. If one or more of the posterior roots of the lumbar spinal nerves are cut or, better still, if the posterior funiculi are severed in this region, the degeneration will affect the exogenous fibers throughout their course to the medulla, and it will be seen that in the cervical region the degen- erated fibers are grouped in the area of the fasciculus gracilis (see Fig. 77). The the gray matter of the 4-* Cervical 7tJtDorsal 2^ Lumbar S-'Lumbi Fig. 77. — Diagrams to show course of upward de- generation of fibers of poste- rior funiculi after section of a number of posterior roots of the nerves forming the lumbosacral plexus. — (Mott.) It will be noted that in the cervical regions the degener- ated area is confined to the fasciculus gracilis. 172 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. endogenous filters, so far as they are ascending, represent afferent paths in which two or more neurons arc concerned. The pos- terior root fibers concerned in these paths end in the gray matter of the cord, and thence the conduction is continued by one or more tract cells. The conduction by this set of fibers may be on the same side of the cord as that on which the root fibers entered, or it may be crossed, or, using a convenient terminology, it may be homolateral or contralateral. The physiological value of the ascending fibers in the posterior funiculi has been investigated by a large number of observers. The physiologists have employed the direct method of cutting the funiculi in the thoracic or lumbar region and observing the effect upon the sensations of the parts below the lesion. The positive results of these experiments have been difficult to discover. Most of the older observers found that there was no detectable change in the sensations of the parts below, but they paid attention only to cutaneous sensations, and, indeed, chiefly to the sense of pain. Later observers* have differed also in their description of the effects of this operation; but most of them state that the animal shows an awkwardness or lack of skill in the movements of the hind limbs, especially in the finer movements, and this effect is interpreted to mean that there is some loss of muscle sense. This conclusion is strength- ened by the results of pathological anatomy. In the disease known as tabes dorsalis the posterior funiculi of the cord in the lu inliar legion are affected and the striking symptom of this condition is an interference with the power of co-ordinating properly t lie movements of the lower limbs, particularly in the ad of maintaining body equilibrium in standing and walking, — a condition known as Locomotor ataxia. So far as the cutaneous .lions are concerned, — that is, the sensations of touch (pressure), pain, and temperature, all observers agree that the two latter are not affected, while regarding touch, opinions have differed radically. Schifif contended that touch sensations are detectable as long as these funiculi are intact and are seriously interfered with when they are sectioned; bu1 most of the results, pathological and experimental, indicate thai when the con- tinuity of these fibers is destroyed the sense of touch is still en1 in the parts supplied by the cord below the lesion. To summarize, therefore, we may say thai the evidence at hand proves that the ascending fihers of the posterior funiculi do not convey impulses of pain or temperature, that if they convey touch (pressure) impulses they certainly do not form the * Borchert. "Archiv f. Physiologic, " L902, 389. Sec also Sherrington, "Journal of Physiology," 14, 255, L893. SPINAL CORD AS A PATH OF CONDUCTION. 173 only path of conduction for this sense, and that most probably their chief function is the conduction of impulses of muscle sense, — that is, they consist of sensory fibers from the voluntary muscles and their tendons. The muscle sensations thus aroused in the higher parts of the brain are necessary to the proper co-ordination of the movements of the muscles. Injury to these funiculi, therefore, while it does not cause paralysis, is followed by disorderly — that is, ataxic — movements. On the histological side it has been shown, as stated above, that the fibers, particularly the exogenous fibers, end in nuclei of the medulla and thence are continued forward by the great sensory tract known as the "lemniscus," to end eventually in that part of the cortex of the cerebrum designated as the area of the body senses. Ascending (Afferent or Sensory) Paths in the Lateral Funic- uli.— The two best known ascending tracts in these funiculi are those of the cerebellospinal and the superficial anterolateral fasciculi. The former takes its origin in the upper lumbar region, and is composed of axons connected with the tract cells of the dorsal nucleus. The impulses which its fibers convey are brought into the cord through those fibers of the posterior root that end around the cells of the dorsal nucleus. A number of the fibers in this funiculus end doubtless in the gray matter of the upper regions of the cord, but most of them continue upward on the same side, enter the inferior peduncle of the cerebellum (rest if or m body), and terminate in the posterior and median portions of the vermiform lobe, mainly on the same side, but partly also on the opposite side. The superficial anterolateral fasciculus, situated ventrally to the cerebellospinal fas- ciculus (gr, Fig. 75), may ex- tend forward into the anterior funiculi along the periphery of the cord. The two bun- dles may be more or less intermingled at the points of contact. This tract begins also in the upper lumbar region, its fibers arising on the same side from tract cells situated in the anterior column and the so-called intermediate portions of the gray matter. Many of the fibers in this tract doubtless terminate in the cord Wave Fig. 78. — To show the course of the fibers of the cerebellar tracts of the cord (Molt): v.a.c, Ventral tract (superficial anterolateral); d.a.c, dorsal tract (cerebellospinal); s.v., superior vermis; P.C.Q., inferior colliculus. 174 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. itself, since the bundle does not increase regularly in size as it passes up the cord. Most of the bundle continues forward, however, along the ventral side of the pons, gradually shifts more to the dorsal side, and at the level of the superior peduncles of the cerebellum turns backward, for the most part at least, and passes to the cerebellum by way of the superior peduncle (brachium conjunctivum) and the anterior medullary velum, to end in the vermiform lobe chiefly on the same side, but to some extent on the opposite side* (Fig. 78). Where this tract separates from the cerebellospinal fasciculus it isstatedf that it gives off a number of fibers which enter the restiform body with the cerebellospinal fasciculus to end in the cerebellum. This and other facts indicate that the two tracts constitute a com- mon system. Regarding the physiology of these two tracts there is little experimental and not much clinical evidence. Some observers have cut the cerebellospinal fasciculus in ani- mals, but with no very obvious effect except again a slight degree of ataxia in the movements below the lesion and some loss of muscular tone. J This result, together with the fact that the bundle ends in the cerebellum, gives reason for be- lieving that the fibers mediate muscular sensibility. As we shall see, much evidence of various kinds connects the cere- bellum with the co-ordination of the muscles of the body in the complex movements of standing and locomotion. This power of co-ordination in turn depends upon the sensory impulses from the muscles and perhaps the joints and other so-called deep sen- sory parts, and since the fibers of the cerebellospinal fasciculus end in the cerebellum, and since experimental lesion of them gives no loss of cutaneous sensibility and some degree of ataxia, ii seems justifiable to conclude that these fibers are physiolog- ically muscle-sense libers. The superficial anterolateral fasciculus has not been the subject of much experimental study from the physiological side. Clinically, the tract may be involved in pai hological or traumal ic lesions of t he lateral funiculi. Gowers § gives a history of some such cases which lead him to believe that this trad constitutes a pathway for pain impulses, and this view or the view thai it conducts the impulses of both pain and temperature has been more or less generally accepted. Little confidence, however, can be placed in this conclusion, since the le ions in question were not strictly confined to the fasciculus ■ For the literature upon these tracts, sec Vim Gehuchten, "Le NeVraxe," 3, 157, 1901. Bchaefer and Bruce, "Journal <>f Physiology," 1907 ("Proc. Physiol. - J Bing, "Archiv fur Physiologie, " 1906, 250. § <;.. ■ Lancet," 1886. SPINAL CORD AS A PATH OF CONDUCTION. 175 in question. The only positive indication that we have con- cerning the physiological value of these fibers is given by then- histology in the fact that they end for the most part in the cerebellum. The cerebellum we know may be removed in dogs and monkeys without loss of the sensation of pain, temperature, or touch, and this fact speaks strongly against the view that either the cerebellospinal or the superficial anterolateral fascicu- lus is concerned in the conduction of these cutaneous sensations. From a physiological standpoint we should be inclined to believe that both of these tracts conduct the impulses of the deeper sensibilities, particularly the sensations arising from the muscles, tendons, and joints. It would seem, therefore, that all the long ascending tracts in the posterior and lateral funiculi of the cord may be made up of fibers of muscle sense, using this term in a wide sense to include the deep sensibility of the joints, tendons, and muscles. The immense importance of muscular sensibility in the maintenance of life and in defense against enemies may explain, upon the doctrine of the struggle for existence, why the long paths should have been developed first in connection with this sense. The Spinal Paths for the Cutaneous Senses (Touch, Pain, and Temperature). — From the facts stated in the last two para- graphs it would seem probable that the spinal paths for touch, pain, and temperature must be along the short association tracts of the proper fasciculi of the lateral and anterior funiculi. There is evidence from the clinical side that the paths of con- duction for these senses are separate. In the pathological condition known as syringomyelia, cavities are formed in the cord affecting chiefly the central gray matter and the contiguous portions of the white. In these cases a frequent symptom is what is known as the dissociation of sensations; the patient loses, in certain regions, the sensations of pain and temperature (analgesia and thermo-anesthesia), but preserves that of pressure (touch). Facts of this kind indicate that the paths of conduc- tion for touch are separate from those for pain and temperature, but little that is positive is known regarding the exact location of these paths. The fibers of pain and temperature probably end in the gray matter of the cord (posterior column) soon after their entrance, and the path is continued upward by tract cells whose axons enter the proper fasciculi in the lateral or anterolateral funiculi, most probably in the lateral funiculi, so far at least as the lower animals are concerned,* but the number of such neurons concerned in the conduction as far as the medulla is not known. Regarding the path for the touch impulses a singular * For discussion, see Bertholet, " Le Nevraxe," 1906, vii., 283. 17G PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. amount of uncertainty prevails. This sense is not lost or, at least, is rarely lost in cases of syringomyelia in which the other cutaneous senses are affected. On the other hand, the posterior funiculi, as we have seen, may be completely sectioned in lower animals without destroying or, indeed, affecting the sense of touch, and in the case of man extensive pathological lesions of the same funiculi are reported in which the sense of touch was not lost. Some authors, therefore, have been led to believe that the touch impulses may be conveyed up the cord by several paths: by the long association fibers of the posterior funiculi and by the short association fibers of the lateral funiculi. Such a view receives little support from the experimental work on the lower mammals. In these animals the evidence tends to show that the conduction is by way of the lateral or anterolateral funiculi, by means of tract cells and short association tracts. The fact that in man the clinical evidence seems to point to the posterior funiculi as a possible or, indeed, probable path for these fibers may serve to exemplify the fact that in these matters the various mammalia differ more or less according to the degree of their development. It seems possible also that so far as man is concerned an explanation of the difference of opinion regard- ing the spinal paths of the sense of touch is found in the distinc- tion made by Head and Thompson* between tactile discrimina- tion and tactile localization. By the former is meant the ability to discriminate between two stimuli applied simultaneously to the skin at a certain distance apart, by the latter the ability to perceive and locate accurately a pressure stimulus applied to the skin. These two forms of cutaneous touch sensations are mediated according to these authors by separate systems of fibers. As the result of a spinal lesion the power of dis- crimination may be h>st over a given area of skin winch other- wise is completely sensitive to all cutaneous stimuli. They find thai the fibers of tactile discrimination travel up the cord uncrossed in the posterior funiculi, together with some of the fibers of muscle sense that is, the fibers which give us a sense of position and movement of the limbs. The libers of tactile local- ization, on the Contrary, pass upward in the lateral or antero- lateral funiculi ami cross to the other side before reaching the medulla. Homolateral and Contralateral Conduction of the Cuta- neous Impulses. < Ireal interest, from the medical side, has been hown in the question of the crossed or uncrossed conduc- tion of the cutaneous impulses in the cord. The matter is nat- urally one of importance in diagnosis. In human beings it was * I lead and Thompson, "I (rain, " 1906, SPINAL CORD AS A PATH OF CONDUCTION. 177 pointed out by Brown-Sequard* that unilateral lesions of the cord are followed by muscular paralysis below on the same side and loss of cutaneous sensibility on the opposite side. This syn- drome has been held clinically to establish the diagnosis of a uni- lateral lesion, and has led to the view that, while the conduction of the motor impulses is homolateral, that of the cutaneous sensory impulses is contralateral. Experimental work on lower animals, on the contrary, has not supported this view. While results in this direction have varied, as would be expected from the intrinsic difficulties connected with the interpretation of the sensations of an animal, the general outcome has been to show that the sensory conduction is bilateral, but mainly on the same side. That is, if the cord is cut on one side only (hemisected) in the thoracic region, the cutaneous sensibility of the parts below the lesion is impaired upon the same side, but not completely abolished, showing that some crossing has taken place, f It is probable that this crossing is more com- plete in man than in the lower animals, although later studies in man of unilateral lesions of the cord (Brown-Sequard paraly- sis) indicate that the contralateral loss of cutaneous sensibility affects chiefly the senses of pain and temperature; the loss of touch is not complete and muscular sensibility, of course, is affected only on the same side. Head and Thompson, in the paper previously referred to, conclude upon the basis of extensive clinical studies that in man all the fibers of cutaneous sense cross in the cord except those mediating tactile discrimination. As stated above, these latter pass upward in the posterior funic- uli together with some of the fibers of muscle sense, and do not cross until after they reach the medulla. According to their interpretation, a complete unilateral lesion of the cord in the cervical region would be followed by a homolateral loss of motion in the parts below and also of tactile discrimination and muscle sense, using the latter term to cover the deep sensibility in regard to position and movements of the limbs. On the contralateral side there would be a loss of pain, temperature, and tactile localization in the skin, and also a loss of deep sen- sibility to touch (see p. 271). The Descending (Efferent or Motor) Paths in the Antero- lateral Column. — The main descending path in the cord is the pyramidal or cerebrospinal system of fibers. In man, as shown in Fig. 75, there are two fasciculi belonging to this system, — the anterior and the lateral pyramidal tracts. Both tracts arise from the anterior pyramids on the ventral face of the * Brown-Sequard, "Journal de Physiologie, " 6, 124, 232, 581, 1863. t Mott, "Brain," 1895, 1, and Bertholet, "Le Nevraxe," 1906, vii., 283. 12 17s PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. medulla, whence the name of the pyramidal system. At the junction of the medulla and curd the fibers of the pyramids decussate in part, forming a conspicuous feature of the interna] structure at this point, known as the pyramidal decussation. According to the general schema of this decussation (see Fig. 79), the larger number of the fibers in the pyramid of one side pass over to form the lateral pyramidal fasciculus of the other side of the cord (4, 5), while a smaller part (3) continues down on the same side to form the anterior pyrami- dal fasciculus. Eventually, how- ever, these latter fibers also cross the mid-line in the anterior white commissure, not, however, all at once, as at the pyramidal decussa- tion, but some at the level of each spinal nerve. These pyramidal fibers have their origin in the cor- tex of the cerebral hemispheres in large pyramidal cells; some of them cross the mid-line before reaching the medulla to end around the colls of origin of the cranial nerves, but the greater number continue into the cord and, after crossing the mid-line in the pyramidal decussation or in the anterior white commissure, terminate around the motor cells of the anterior columns which give rise to the motor roots of the spinal nerves. Both fasciculi, the lateral and the anterior, con- tinue throughout, the length of t he cord, diminishing in area on the way as some of their fibers terminate in each segment. This system of libers is supposed to represent the mechanism for effecting voluntary movements, and according to the general schema the voluntary motor path from cerebrum to muscle comprises two neurons, the pyra- midal or cerebrospinal neuron and the spinal or the cranial neuron. Moreover, as represented in the schema, the innerva- tion is crossed, the right side of the brain controlling the wus- FIk. 70. Schema representing the course ol the fibers of tin- pyra- midal or cerebrospinal system: I, Fibers to the nuclei of t Uo cranial nei i '■; 2, uncrossed Bbei - to I he lateral pyramidal fasciculus; •''.. fibers to iii<- anterior pyramidal fs ciculu ' in the cord; t and 5, fibers that oro i in the pyramidal decu a tion to male the lateral pyramidal lu of I In- opposite Jide. SPINAL CORD AS A PATH OF CONDUCTION. 179 culature of the left side of the body and vice versa. As we shall see, however, when we come to study the motor areas of the brain, this rule has important exceptions, and histologically there is proof that some of the fibers in each pyramid (2 in Fig. 79) continue into and terminate in the cord on the same side. The pyramidal system varies, in an interesting way, in the extent of its development among the different vertebrates. It reaches its highest development in man and the anthropoid apes. In the other mammalia it is relatively less important and the anterior fasciculus in the anterior funiculus is lacking altogether. In the birds what represents the same system is found in the anterior funiculus (Sandmeyer), while in the frog the system does not exist at all. The relative importance of the system in the different mammalia is indicated in the accompanying table taken from Lenhossek,* in which the area of the pyramidal system is given in percentage of the total cross-area of the cord: Mouse 1.14 per cent. Guinea pig 3.0 " Rabbit 5.3 Cat 7.76 Man 11.87 Evidently, therefore, the importance of the pyramidal system varies in different animals, and it is necessary to bear this fact in mind in applying the results of experiments on the lower animals to man. In the lowest vertebrates there are undoubtedly motor paths between the brain and cord through which so-called voluntary movements are effected, but these are probably short paths in- volving a number of neurons. The higher the position of the animal in the phylogenetic scale, the more complete is the develop- ment of the long pyramidal system; but even in the higher mam- mals it is probable that motor paths, other than the pyramidal system, connect the cortex and subcortical centers with the motor nuclei in the cord. In the dog, for example, section of the pyramids is not followed by complete paralysis, and, indeed, after such sections stimulation of the motor areas of the cortex still causes definite muscular movements. f One such indirect motor path is referred to below in connection with the rubrospinal tract (Monakow's bundle) . Less Well-Known Tracts in the Cord. — In addition to the tracts just described there are a number of others — mainly, descend- ing tracts — concerning which our anatomical knowledge is less complete, and the physiological value of which is entirely un- * Lenhossek, " Bau des Nervensystems," second edition, 1895. t Rothmann, " Zeitschrift f. klin. med.," vol. xlviii., 1903. ISO PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. known or at best is a matter of inference from the anatomical relations.* Descending Tracts in the Posterior Funiculus — Comma Tract; Oval Field. — In the posterior funiculi several tracts of descending fibers have been described. The comma tract of Schultze is found in the cervical and the upper thoracic cord. The bundle lies at the border-line between the fasciculus gracilis and the fasciculus cuneatus. In the lower regions of the cord, lumbar and sacral, similar small areas of descending fibers are found — oval field (Flechsig), median triangle (Gombault and Philippe) — which represent possibly different systems. It is probable that these fibers belong to the group of long association fibers connecting distant portions of the cord. Nothing is known regarding their physiology. Descending Tracts in the Anterolateral Funiculus. — The pre- pyramidal tract, known also as Monakow's bundle, the fasciculus intermediolateralis, or the rubrospinal tract, is a conspicuous bundle forming a wedge-shaped or triangular area in the lateral columns between the lateral pyramidal fasciculus and the superficial anterolateral fasciculus (Gower's), or, perhaps, more correctly speaking, forming the anterior portion of the lateral pyramidal fasciculus; the two systems being more or less inter- mingled. The fibers composing this bundle are descending fibers that take their origin in the midbrain in the cells of the red nucleus. .Shortly after their origin they cross to the opposite side and, passing through the pons and medulla, enter the spinal cord in the lateral funiculi, in which they may be detected as far as the sacral region. Its fibers terminate around cells lying in the posterior part of the anterior column of gray matter, whose axons, in 1 urn, probably emerge through the anterior roots. This tract, therefore, constitutes a crossed motor path from midbrain to the anterior roots, and, since the red nucleus, in turn, is con- nected with the cerebrum, either directly or by way of the cerebellum, it represents a cerebrospinal motor path in addition to thai offered by the pyramidal System. The vestibulospinal fibers lie anterior to the preceding tract in the anterolateral funiculus; they may extend into the anterior funiculus as far as the .'interior pyramidal fasciculus. These fibers originate in the nucleus of Deiters. In the cord the fibers end around cells in the anterior column. Since the Deiters nucleus forms a termination for the sensory fibers of the vestibu- lar branch of the eighth cranial nerve, and since these fibers are ♦ Collier and Buzzard, "Brain," L001, 177; and Fraser, "Journal of Physiology," 28, 866, 1902. For Bummary and Literature, consult: Van Ge- hucbten, " Anatomie du systeme aerveux '!<■ I'homme," 4th ed., L906. SPINAL CORD AS A PATH OF CONDUCTION. 181 believed to give us a sense of the position of the body and to be concerned in the reflex adjustment of the muscles in the move- ments used to maintain equilibrium, their connection in Deiters' nucleus with a spinal motor path becomes very significant as furnishing a reflex arc through which sensory impressions from the vestibular apparatus in the ear may automatically control the musculature of the body. A number of other descending paths in the anterior and lateral funiculi have been described, such as Helweg's bundle or the olivospinal tract, lying on the margin of the cord at the junction of the anterior and the lateral funiculi and supposed to arise in the olivary bodies; the anterior and the lateral reticulospinal tracts arising from cells in the reticular formation of medulla, pons, and midbrain; and the continuation into the cord of the important medial longitudinal fasciculus (post. long, bundle), which extends from the midbrain through to the cord and connects the motor nuclei of the cranial nerves with the motor centers of the cord. Concerning these and similar tracts our physiological knowledge is scanty, and it is not possible at present to employ them with certainty in explaining the activity of the neuromuscular apparatus. CHAPTER IX. THE GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF THE CEREBRUM AND ITS MOTOR FUNCTIONS. From the time of Galen in the second century of the Christian era the brain has been recognized as the organ of intelligence and conscious sensations. Galen established this view not only by anatomical dissections, confirming the older work of the Alexandrian school (third century B.C.) in regard to the origin from the brain of the cranial nerves, but also by numerous vivisection experiments upon lower animals. All modern work has confirmed this belief and has tended to show that in the cerebral hemispheres and, indeed, in the cortex of gray matter lies the seat of consciousness. It is perhaps still an open question as to the existence of a conscious or psychical factor in the activities of other parts of the nervous system, but there is no doubt that the highest develop- ment of psychical activity in man is associated with the cortical mat- ter of the cerebrum. In the young infant the dawn of its mental powers is connected with and dependent on the development of the normal cortical structure, while in extreme age the failure in the mental faculties goes hand in hand with an atrophy of the elements of the cortex. If this cortex were removed all the intelligence, sen- sation, and thought that we recognize as characterizing the highest psychical life of man would be destroyed, and abnormalities in the structure of this cortical material are accepted as the immediate causal factor of those perversions in reasoning and in character which are exhibited by the insane or the degenerate. The cortical gray matter, therefore, is the chief organ of the psychical life, the tissue through whose activity the objective? changes in the externa] world, so far as they affect our sense organs, are converted into the subjective changes of consciousness. The nature of this reac- tion constitutes the most difficult problem of physiology and psy- chology, a problem which it is generally believed is beyond the possibility of a satisfactory scientific explanation. For if is held that the methods of science are applicable only to the investiga- tion of the objective that is, the physical and chemical change wit hin t he nervous mat ter, while i he psychical reaction is of a nature that cannol be approached through the conceptions or methods of physical science. In other words, then; is a- physicochemical IS2 GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF THE CEREBRUM. 183 mechanism in the brain matter which is capable of giving us a reaction in consciousness. The methods of physiology are adapted to the investigation of the nature of this mechanism, but the reac- tion in consciousness deals with a something which so far as we know is not matter or energy, and which, therefore, is not within the scope of physiological or, indeed, scientific explanation. In what follows, therefore, attention is called only to the mechanical side, — the facts that have been discovered regarding the anatomical structure and the physical and chemical properties of the nervous mechanism. The Histology of the Cortex. — The finer structure of the different regions of the cortex has been the subject of much investi- gation, but in this connection it is only necessary to recall the elementary facts so far as they are useful in physiological explana- tions. Leaving aside minor differences in the shape and stratifica- tion of the cells, it is an interesting fact that the cortex everywhere has a similar structure. It consists of four or five layers more or less clearly distinguishable (see Fig. 80). 1. The molecular layer, lying immediately beneath the pia mater, and having a thickness of about 0.25 mm. In this layer, in addition to the supporting neuroglia, there are found a number of very small nerve cells of several types lying with their processes parallel to the surface of the brain. The axons and dendrites of these small cells terminate within the layer, so that they take no direct part in the formation of the white matter of the brain, but have, so to speak, a distributive or associative function. In this layer, also, end many of the dendrites of the larger nerve cells of the deeper layers and the terminal arborization of entering nerve fibers (axons) from other regions. It must be conceived, there- fore, as containing a fine feltwork of nerve fibrils, — dendrites and axons or their collaterals, — and as a region, therefore, in which many of the incoming impulses along afferent fibers are, so to speak, distributed into outgoing ones. On histological grounds Cajal was inclined to believe that this layer represents the location of the most important psychical reactions. 2. The layer of small pyramidal cells of about the same thick- ness as the last. This layer contains a number of small nerve cells, mostly of the pyramidal type, with the apex directed toward the external surface. The dendrites from the apical process termi- nate in the molecular layer, while the axon arising from the basal side of the cell passes inwardly to constitute one of the nerve fibers of the medullary portion of the cerebrum. 3. The layer of large pyramidal cells. This layer, much thicker (1 mm.) than the preceding, is not sharply differentiated from it. It contains large pyramidal cells, particularly in the Rolandic area. 184 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. White Mali iir hu. To show the ttructure <.f the cortea oere- bri r/^, /, rine) : /, The molec- ular layer; //, the layer of vertical fu iform eel] ; ///, thr- layer of small pyramidal cell : / V , i he l- i , the luyor of polymorphic cell ■ Their form and connections are. in general, the same as those given for the small pyramidal cells. 4. The layer of fusiform or polymor- phic nerve cells. A small layer of cells whose form is more irregular than that of the pyramidal cells, but whose axons also pass into the medullary portion of the cerebrum, while their dendrites stretch externally into the layers of pyramidal cells. In this layer are found also some cells belonging to the second type of Golgi (Martinotti cells). The axis cylin- der processes from these latter cells, in- stead of becoming medullated fibers of the white matter of the cerebrum, pass toward the external surface, to end in the pyramidal or molecular layer in a number of minute branches. 5. The medulla of the cerebrum. The white matter of the cerebrum begins immediately below the last-named layer, and consists (1) of nerve fibers which originate from the pyramidal and poly- morphic cells immediately exterior to it, and which carry outgoing impulses from that part of the cortex, and (2) of fibers arising elsewhere in the cortex or in the lower portions of the brain, which ter- minate in the cortex and carry the incoming impulses, — impulses which are afferent as regards that part of the cor- tex. I The fibers in this white matter may be classified under three heads: First, the projection system {A, B, C, D, and E of Fig. SI), comprising those filters, afferent and efferent, which connect the cortex with underlying parts of the cen- tral nervous system, — the spinal cord, medulla, pons, midbrain, or thalamus. I'hi-: great projection system emerges, for the most pari, through the internal cap- sule and t he peduncles of t lie cerebrum. Certain parts Of t lie cortex .are seemingly lacking in a projection system; the fibers GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF THE CEREBRUM. 185 Fig. 81. — Schema of the projection fibers of the cerebrum and of the peduncles of the cerebellum; lateral view of the internal capsule : A, Tract from the frontal gyri to the pons nuclei, and so to the cerebellum (frontal cerebro-cortico-pontal tract) ; B, the motor (pyramidal) tract ; C, the sensory (lemniscus) tract ; D, the visual tract ; E, the auditory tract ; F, the fibers of the superior peduncle of the cerebellum ; G, fibers of the middle pedun- cle uniting with A in the pons ; H, fibers of the inferior peduncle of the cerebellum ; J, fibers between the auditory nucleus and the inferior colliculus ; K, motor (pyramidal) decussation in the bulb ; Vt, fourth ventricle. The numerals refer to the cranial nerves. — (Modified from Starr.) Fig. 82. — Lateral view of a human hemisphere, showing the bundles of association fibers (Starr): A, A, Between adjacent gyri; B, between frontal and occipital areas; C, between frontal and temporal areas, cingulum ; D. between frontal and temporal areas, fasciculus uncinatus ; E, between occipital and temporal areas, fasciculus longitudinalis inferior ; C.N, caudate nucleus ; O.T, thalamus. 186 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. arising from these parts do not enter the capsule to make connec- tion with the motor and sensory paths, below, but pass to other parts of the cortex, forming a part of the system of association fibers. Second, the association system, which may be defined as comprising those fibers which connect one part of the cortex with another (Fig. si1'. There are short association tracts (A, A) con- necting neighboring convolutions and long tracts passing from one lobe to another. Third, the conimissural system, consisting of as- sociation fibers that cross the mid-line and connect portions of one cerebral hemisphere with the cortex of the other. These fibers make up the commissural bands known in gross anatomy as the corpus callosum, anterior white commissure, fornix, etc. Physiological Deductions from the Histology of the Cortex. — Cajal* especially lays stress upon some anatomical features which seem to justify certain generalizations of a physiological nature. In the first place, every part of the cortex receives incoming impulses and gives rise to outgoing impulses. Every part of the cortex is, therefore, both a termination of some afferent path and the begin- ning of some efferent path; it is, in other words, a reflex arc of a greater or less degree of complexity. We may suppose that every efferent discharge from any part of the cortex is occasioned by afferent impressions reaching that point from some other part of the nervous system. Whether or not there is such a thing as absolutely spontaneous mental activity cannot be determined by physiology, but on the anatomical side at least all the structures exhibit connections that fit them for reflex stimulation, and many of our apparently spontaneous acts must be of this character. Secondly, all parts of the cortex exhibit an essentially similar structure. Modern physiology has recognized clearly that different parte of the cerebrum have different functions, but the differentia- tion in structure which usually accompanies a specialization in function is not very evident. Definite differences in the thickness of the layers, in the size or shape of the cells, have been pointed out, bul it is perhaps omething of a disappointment, to find so little of an anatomical distinction between structures whose reaction in consciousness is so widely separated. It would seem that the structural peculiarities must lie chiefly in the ultimate chemical composition and physical properties of the protoplasm. In the third place, the central nervous system throughout \\)v verte- brates i constructed upon the same lines, a mechanism of intercon- necting neurons. There is a vast difference in the mental activity of a frog and a man, but the cortex of the cerebrum shows a funda- mental •iiiiil.-irii ■ in :-i met ure in the two cases. The chief difference thai comparative anatomy is able to show is that iu the higher ' I [,i i .,u .iii mI.y bui la structure du systeme nerveux, etc.," Paris, is!) t. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF THE CEREBRUM. 187 animals the greater mental development is associated with a greater complexity and richness in the connections of the neurons. As shown in Figs. 83 and 84, the number of processes, particularly the dendritic processes, is much greater in the cortical cells of the higher animals; or, to put this fact in another way, the number of cells in the cortex of the higher animals is much less for an area of the same size than in lower animals. The amount of in-between substance or the richness of the network of processes Fig. 83.- — A-D, Showing the phylogenetic development of mature nerve cells in a series of vertebrates: a-e, the ontogenetic development of growing cells in a typical mam- mal (in both cases only pyramidal cells from the cerebrum are shown); A, frog; B, lizard; C, rat ; D, man ; a, neuroblast without dendrites ; 6, commencing dendrites ; c, dendrites further developed; d, first appearance of collateral branches; e, further development of collaterals and dendrites. — (From Ramon y Cajal.) is increased. This anatomical fact would indicate that the greater mental activity in the higher animals is dependent, in part, upon the richer interconnection of the nerve cells, or, expressed physio- logically, our mental processes are characterized by their more numerous and complex associations. A visual or auditory stim- ulus that, in the frog, for instance, may call forth a comparatively simple motor response, may in man, on account of the numerous associations with the memory records of past experiences, lead to 1SS PHYSIOLOGY OV CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. psychical and motor responses of a much more intricate and in- direct character. Extirpation of the Cerebrum.— One of the methods used in physiology to determine the general functional value of the cerebral hemispheres has been to remove them completely, by surgical operation, and to study the effect upon the psychical responses of the animal. Upon the cold-blooded animals and the birds the operation may be performed with ease, but in these animals the positive results are not striking and the experiments are valuable 1 ' " - ' l \ . '•' i .-), >' ' » • . .,'■> »■;•».. ' » .'' '■::'/'!'. .'■v. ,1 rv .»-.. * *• /« . ..'1 if * !' *. .) ,'' ■■ fl ?> c £ig, B4. Section through cone ponding pari i <>( the cortex in: a, Man; f», «; and e, mole, to show the greater eparation o! the aerve cells in the higher animals. - {Rethe, after chiefly for their negative results. II' the cerebral hemispheres are removed from the frog, for example, the animal after recovering from the immediate effects of the operation that is, the effects of the anesthetic and the shock shows surprisingly little difference from the normal animal, tt maintains a normal posture and shows no loss at all in its power of equilibration. When placed on its bach it quickly regains its usual position, [f thrown into water GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF THE CEREBRUM. 189 it swims to a solid support and crawls out like a normal animal. It jumps when stimulated and is careful to avoid obstacles placed in its way, showing that its visual reflexes are not impaired. It is said, however, that the more complicated reactions that depend upon the memory of past experiences or the instincts are absent or imperfect. This latter peculiarity is manifested most impressively in birds (pigeons) after removal of a part or all of the cerebrum. As a result of such an operation, the nervous, active animal is changed at once to a stupid, lethargic creature which reacts only when stimulated. It sits in a drowsy attitude, with its head drawn in to the shoulders, its eyes closed, and its feathers slightly erected; occasionally it will open its eyes, stretch the neck, gape, preen its feathers perhaps, and then sink back into its somnolent attitude. The animal in this condition maintains its equilibrium perfectly, flies well if thrown into the air and perches comfortably upon a narrow support. It may be kept alive apparently indefinitely by appropriate feeding and so long as it is well fed retains its stupid and impassive appearance. If allowed to starve for a while it becomes restless from the effects of hunger, may walk to and fro, and peck aimlessly at the ground. If surrounded by grain it may peck at the separate grains, but never actually seizes one in its beak and swallows it. The striking defect in these animals is the loss of those responses that depend upon memory of past or in- herited experiences. Its motor reactions are all of a simple kind. If placed upon a hot plate it will, for a time, lift first one foot, then the other, and finally squat, but never flies away. When dosing a loud noise awakens it, but it exhibits no signs of fear, and quickly relapses into somnolence when the auditory stimulus ceases. The one positive conclusion that we may draw from the behavior of these animals is that in them the cerebrum is the place in which the memory records are stored, and that when it is removed the actions of the animal when stimulated become much more direct and predictable, since the stimulus awakens no associations with past experiences. The complete removal of the cerebrum in mam- mals is attended with more difficulty. When taken out at once by a single operation the animal survives but a short time and the permanent effects of the operation cannot be detected. Goltz,* however, has succeeded, in dogs, in removing by a peculiar operation all of the cerebral cortex. The operation was performed in sev- eral successive stages with an interval of several months between. In the most successful experiment the animal was kept alive for a year and a half and the postmortem examination showed that all of the cortex had been removed except a small portion of the tip of the temporal lobe, and this latter, since its connection with * Goltz, " Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie," 51, 570, 1892. 190 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. the other parts of the brain had been destroyed, was, of course, functionless. In addition, a large part of the corpora striata and the thalami and a small portion of the midbrain had been re- moved. The behavior of this animal was studied carefully. After the immediate effects of the operation — paralysis, etc. — had disap- peared the animal moved easily ; in fact, showed a tendency to keep moving continually. There was no permanent paralysis of the so- culled voluntary movements. He answered to sensory stimuli of various kinds, but not in an intelligent way. If, for instance, a painful stimulus was applied to the skin, he would growl or bark, and turn his head toward the place stimulated; but did not attempt to bite. Xo caressing could arouse signs of pleasure, and no threatening signs of fear or anger. Like the pigeon, the most con- spicuous defect in the animal was a lack of intelligent response,— that is, the responses to sensory stimuli were simple, and evidently did not involve complex associations with past experiences. His memory records, for the most part, had been destroyed. Goltz records that when starved he showed signs of hunger, and that eventually he learned to feed himself when his nose was brought into contact with the food, although he was not able to recognize food placed near him. He would reject food with a disagreeable taste. When sleeping he gave no signs of dreaming, differing in this respect from normal dogs. Localization of Functions in the Cerebrum. — When the belief was established that the cerebrum is the organ of the higher psychical activities there arose naturally the question whether dif- ferent parts of the cortex have different functions corresponding to the various faculties of the mind, or whether the cerebrum is functionally equivalent throughout, in the same sense, for instance, as the liver. This question of the localization of functions in the brain (cerebrum) has been much debated, but the most interesting ami important discussions upon the subject belong to the nine- teen) li century. About the beginning of the century Franz Joseph Gall, at that time a physician in Vienna, began to teach publicly his well-known system of cranioscopy or, as it- was later designated by hfa chief disciple (Spurzheim), system of phrenology.* (Jail, from his earl;.- youth, was possessed with the idea that the different faculties of the mind are mediated through different parts of the brain, that in it we have to deal not with a single, but with a plurality of Organs. This belief was in opposition to the eiirrenl ideas of his times and Gall devoted his entire life to an earnest effort to estab- lish and popularize his views. He and his disciples contributed many very important facts to our knowledge of the finer anatomy of the brain; but, so far as the view of separate organs in the II (and Spurzheim), " Recherche* hi ir In systeme w-rvcux en ^'neml .1 celtu AnjusSfVaguia- " Sulcus SCen Abdotnett- Ovest Ear / / 7 Eyelid / Qosun Sulcus Centralis ua / Closure , \\~ ±*S <£>ulcusl Nose °fjai° Obenmg Voea,L Mastication, ofiOLM cords Fig. 86. — Location of motor areas in brain of chimpanzee. — {Sherrington and Green- baum.) The extent of the motor areas is indicated by stippling; it lies entirely in front of the fissure of Rolando (sulcus centralis). Much of the motor area is hidden in the sulci. The regions marked eyes indicate the areas whose stimulation gives conjugate movements of the eyeballs. It is doubtful, however, whether these represent motor areas proper. careful stimulation of the region in monkeys was more or less verified upon man, since in operations upon the brain it was often necessary to stimulate the cortex in order to localize a given motor area. By these means charts have been made showing the cortical area for the musculature of each part of the body. It was found that in general the distribution of the areas lies along the central sulcus of Rolando and follows the order of the cranial and spinal nerves. Within each area smaller centers may be located by careful stimulation; thus, the hand and arm area may be subdivided into centers for the wrist, fingers, thumb, etc. More recently Sherrington and 13 194 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. Greenbaum,* making use of electrical stimulation, unipolar method, have explored carefully the motor areas in the monkey. They state that these areas do not extend back of the central sulcus, but lie chiefly along the anterior central convolution, as represented in Figs. S6 and 87, extending for only a small distance on to the mesial surface of the cerebrum. The area thus delimited by physiological experiments is the region from which arises the pyramidal system of fibers, and clin- ical experience has shown that lesions in this part of the cortex are accompanied by a paralysis of the muscles on the other Sale. CenCrvl. AntJS, * Vagina. Sulacalloso \ ^ Sulc.precenCr.rnarg. marg.-* Sulc.parieCo occip. Sulccalcarin C.S.S. del. Fit;. 87. — To show extension of motor areas on to the mesial .surface, brain of chim- panzee. [Sherrington and Greenbaum). Mesial surface of left hemisphere: Stippled region marked I, K (J gives the motor area for lower limb; /, «, and h .indicate regions from which movements were obtained occasionally with strong stimuli; /, foot ami leg; «, shoulder and chest ; A, thumb and lingers. I he shaded area marked 10 Y K S indicates a region stimulation of which give- conjugate movements of the eyes. side, particularly in the limbs. Pathological or experimental lesions here, moreover, are followed by a degeneration of the pyramidal neurons, — a degeneration which extends to the ter-. ruination of the neurons in the cord. With these data, we can con- struct ;i fairly complete account of the mechanism of voluntary movements. The Initial outgoing or efferent impulses arise in the large pyramidal cells of the motor areas and proceed along the of their neuron- to the motor nuclei of the cranial or spinal nerves. The neurons of the pyramidal tract constitute the motor trad for voluntary* movements; a lesion anywhere along this tract. Causes paralysis, more or less complete and on the other side of the body in general, if the lesion is anterior to the decussation. ♦ "Report of the Thompson Yates and John on Laboratories," 4, 351, 1902; 5, 55, 1903. GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF THE CEREBRUM. 195 The path of the motor fibers is represented in the schema given in Fig. 88. Arising in the cortex, they take the following route (see also Fig. 81, B): 1. Corona radiata. 2 Internal capsule. 3. Peduncle of cerebrum. 4. Pons Varolii, in which they are broken into a number of smaller bundles by the fibers of the middle peduncle of the cerebellum (brachium pontis). In this region, also, some of the fibers cross the mid-line, to end in the motor nuclei of the cranial nerves: Third, fourth, fifth, sixth, and seventh. 5. Anterior pyramids. 6. Pyramidal decussation. 7. Anterior and lateral pyramidal fasciculi in the cord. After ending in the motor nuclei of the cranial or spinal nerves the path is continued by a second neuron from these nuclei to the mus- cles. The entire path involves, therefore, two neurons, and injury to either will cause paralysis of the corresponding muscles. Difference in the Paralysis from Injury to the Spinal and the Pyramidal Neuron. — With regard to the musculature of the limbs especially a difference has been observed in the paralysis caused by injury to the spinal and pyramidal (cerebrospinal) neurons, respectively. Lesions of the anterior root cells in the cord or of the axons arising from them cause complete paratysis of the corresponding muscles, since these muscles are then re- moved not only from voluntary control, but also from reflex effects. The muscles are entirely relaxed and in time exhibit a more or less complete atrophy. When the pyramidal neurons are affected, as in the familiar condition of hemiplegia resulting from a unilateral lesion of the motor cortex, there is paralysis as regards voluntary control, but, the spinal neuron being intact, the muscles are still subject to reflex stimulation through the cord, especially to the so-called tonic impulses. Under these conditions, especially if the lesion is in the cord, it is frequently noticed that the paralyzed muscles are thrown into a state of continuous contraction, contracture, in which they exhibit a spastic rigidity. This fact, therefore, may be used in diagnosing the general location of the lesion. A satisfactory explanation of the cause of the contraction has not been furnished. It may be due to uncontrolled reflex excitation of the spinal neurons, or, as suggested by Van Gehuchten, to the action of the indirect motor path by way of the rubrospinal tract (fasciculus intermediolateralis). Is the Pyramidal System the Only Means of Voluntary (Cor- tical) Control of the Muscles? — Much discussion has arisen 196 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. regarding this question. It is, in fact, one of those questions of nervous mechanism in which experiments upon lower animals must be applied with caution to the conditions in man. As we have seen, the entire cerebral cortex may be removed from the frog, the pigeon, and the dog without causing permanent paralysis, although in the animal last named there is at first a more or less marked loss of voluntary control. But in man and the higher types of the monkey the pyramidal system is more completely devel- oped, and corresponding with this fact it is found that the paralysis from lesion of the motor cortex is more permanent. In fact, observa- tions upon men in whom it has been necessary to remove parts of the motor area by surgical opera- tion indicate that the voluntary control of the muscle is lost or im- paired permanently. It would seem, therefore, that even in an animal as high in the scale as the dog volun- tary control of the muscles can be maintained through fibers other than those belonging to the pyra- midal system. A system such as that found in the rubrospinal tract (p. 180) may be considered as ade- quate to fulfil such ii function. In man, however, along with the more complete development of the pyr- amidal system, the efficacy of the phylogenetically older motor sys- tems is correspondingly reduced. The Crossed Control of the Muscles and Bilateral Represen- tation in the Cortex.— It has been known from very ancient times that an injury to the brain on one side is accompanied by a paralysis of voluntary movement on the other side of the body, a condition known as hemiplegia. The facta given above regarding the origin and course of the pyramidal system of libers explain the crossed character of of tlie paralysis quite satisfactorily. The schema thus pre- sented to us is, however, not entirely without exception. In Fij?. 88.- Schema representing the course of the fibers of the pyra- midal system: L, Fibers to the nuclei of the cranial nerve ; 2, uncrossed fibei to the lateral pyramidal fasciculus ; ■',, fibers <" the anterior pyramidal Itl ii.. ii, I- in I be curd ; I and 6, fibers thai <-m-- m the pyramidal decu ation to make the lateral pyramidal tract of the opposite ide, GENERAL PHYSIOLOGY OF THE CEREBRUM. 197 cases of hemiplegia in which the whole motor area of one side is included it is known that the paralysis on the other side does not involve all the muscles, and, in the second place, it is said that there is some muscular weakness on the same side. The paralysis in hemiplegia affects but little, if at all, those muscles of the trunk which are accustomed to act in unison, — the muscles of inspiration, for instance, the diaphragm, abdominal and intercostal muscles, and the muscles of the larynx. It would appear that these muscles are bilaterally represented in the cortex; so that if one side of the brain is intact the muscles of both sides are still under voluntary control. The mechanism of this bilateral representation is not definitely known; one may conceive several possibilities. The motor area on each side may send down a double set of pyramidal fibers, one of which crosses and the other remains on the same side, or the fibers may bifurcate. Or it is possible that the bilateral control is due to commissural connections between the lower centers in the cord. Some evidence in favor of the former view is found in the undoubted histological fact brought out by Melius and others, that small unilateral lesions in the motor area — the center of the great toe in the monkey, for instance — are followed by degeneration in the lateral pyramidal fasciculus in the cord on both sides, show- ing that some portions of the motor area send fibers to both sides of the body. In cases of hemiplegia it may be added that the muscles of the limbs are not all equally affected. Are the Motor Areas Only Motor in Function? — The great number of nerve cells in the cortex in addition to the large pyramidal cells that give origin to the fibers of the pyramidal system make it possible histologically that other functions may be mediated in the same region. This possibility has been kept in view since the early experiments of Munk, in which he showed that lesions in the Rolandic region are followed by disturbances in what are designated as the body sensations, that is, in muscular and cutaneous sensibility, but especially the former. It was suggested, therefore, at one time that one and the same spot in the cortex might serve as the origin of the motor impulses to a given muscle and as the cortical termination of the sensory impulses coming from the same muscle, the reaction in con- sciousness, the muscular sensations, being mediated perhaps through cells other than those giving rise to the pyramidal fibers. Recent physiological and clinical work has, however, not tended to support this view. The motor areas appear to be confined to the region in front of the central sulcus of Rolando, while the cortical area, which gives rise to that kind of consciousness that we designate in general as body sensibility, extends back of 198 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. this sulcus in the posterior central convolution. Whether, on the other hand, the sense areas for the body (cutaneous and muscular) extend forward into the cortex of the frontal lobe is not clearly shown by experimental or clinical evidence. Flechsig, from his studies upon the time of myelinization of the afferent fibers in the embryo brain, concludes that this is the case, and that, therefore, the motor and sensory areas overlap for a part at least of their extent (see p. 222 and Fig. 97). On the con- trary, in an interesting report by Cushing* of two cases in which the anterior central convolution was stimulated in conscious patients, it is stated that there was no sensation other than that arising from the change in position of the muscles which were thrown into contraction. In the motor area there arc numerous connections by afferent fibers, association tracts, with other parts of the brain. By this means the motor area, without doubt, is brought into relation with many other parts of the cortex, and the sensations or perceptions aroused elsewhere may react upon the motor paths. A voluntary movement, however simple it may be, is a psychological act of some com- plexity, that is to say, every movement is preceded or accom- panied by certain sensations and perceptions which depend upon sensory stimulations occurring at that time, or upon experiences derived from conditions of excitation that have occurred at some previous period — every action is part of a train of conscious or subconscious processes whose neural medi- an ism extends over wide regions of the cortex. The mental processes, the associations, that lead to and originate the motor discharge, cannot be definitely located in the cortex, but the immediate origin of the motor impulse lies, most probably, in the area along the anterior margin of the central sulcus of Rolando, which constitute the foci, so to speak, in which all accessory processes are gathered, so far as they affect our mus- cular acts, and from which emerge the actual efferent stimuli to the different muscles. * Cushing, "American Journal of Physiology," L909 ("Proc. Amer. Physiol. Boc. )• CHAPTER X. THE SENSE AREAS AND THE ASSOCIATION AREAS OF THE CORTEX, The delimitation of the sensory areas in the cortex is a matter of very considerable difficulty, owing partly to the fact that the determination of the presence or absence of certain states of con- sciousness in the animal or person under observation cannot be made except by indirect means, and partly no doubt to the fact that the organization of the sensory mechanism in the brain is more complex and diffuse than in the case of the motor apparatus. Moreover, the distinction between what we may call simple sensa- tions and the more complex psychical representations and judg- ments of which these sensations form a necessary constituent can- not be made clearly, even by the individual in whom the reactions occur. We recognize in ourselves different stages in the degree of consciousness aroused by sensory reactions. Our visual and auditory sensations are clearly differentiated; but many of the lower senses escape recognition in the individual himself, since the state of consciousness accompanying them is of a lower order. Our muscular sensations, for instance, are so indefinite as to be practically subconscious. They are most important to us in every act of our lives, yet the uninformed person is unconscious of the existence of such a sensation, and if deprived of it would recognize the defect only in the consequent loss of control of the voluntary muscular movements. In the attempts to determine in what part ' of the brain the various sensations are mediated every possible method of inquiry has been used : the anatomical course of the sensory paths, physiological experiments of stimulation and ablation, and observations upon individuals with pathological or traumatic lesions in the brain. In the long run, the study of neuropatholog- ical cases in man must give us the last word, because in such cases the estimate of the sensory defect can be made with most accuracy and because in man the specialization of the psychical functions has reached its highest development. The results that have been obtained are perhaps the most definite in the case of the higher senses, vision and hearing, since defects in these senses are recog- nized most clearly, and the anatomical mechanisms involved have proved to be more accessible to investigation. 199 200 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. The Body -sense Area. — In his early experiments Mimk insisted that lesions of the cortex involving the area around the central sulcus are accompanied by a state of anesthesia on the other side of the body, hemianesthesia, particularly as regards the tactile and muscular sensations. It is not necessary, perhaps, to go into the details of the long controversy that arose in connection with this point. Both the clinical and the experi- mental evidence has been contradictory in the hands of different observers, but the tendency of recent studies has been to show, as stated above, that, whereas the motor areas lie anterior to the central sulcus, the sensory areas concerned with the cutaneous and muscular sensations extend posterior to this sulcus.* Posi- tive cases are recorded in which lesions involving the anterior central convolutions were accompanied by paralysis on the other side, hemiplegia, without any detectable disturbance of sensibility, and, on the other hand, lesions have been described in the posterior central and neighboring parietal convolutions in which there was a hemianesthesia more or less distinctly marked without any paralysis. As stated above, Cushing,f in his report upon the stimulation of the cortex in two conscious patients, states that no sensations were aroused by stimuli applied to the anterior central convolution, while stimulation of the posterior convolution aroused distinct sensations of numbness and of touch. Such cases tend to support the view that the motor and body sense areas, although contiguous, do not overlap. Regarding the sensory defects associated with lesions of the parietal lobe posterior to the central sulcus (pos- terior central convolution, supramarginal, superior, and possibly inferior parietal convolutions), it seems probable that they involve chiefly the muscular sense, pressure and temperature sense, and the judgments or perceptions based upon these sensations, while the sense of pain is affected but little, if at all. Monakow gives the order in which sensory defects manifest themselves after such lesions, as follows: The localizing space and muscle sense-, are chiefly affected, in fact, almost lost, on the opposite side; the temperature and pressure sense may be affected, while the pain sense is retained or but slightly affected. The clinicians have observed that the most positive and invaria- ble symptom of lesions in this region is a condition of asfereogno- sis,that is, a diminution in the stereognostic sense or perception. By the stereognostic perception is meant the power to judge concerning the form and consistency of external objects when handled, and it musl be regarded as a perception based upon * Consult Monakow, "Ergebnisse der Physiologic, " 1902, vol. i, part i, p, 821. t Cushing, loc. cil , SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 201 localized sensations of touch and temperature in combination with muscular sensibility. On the whole, therefore, we must infer that the cortex in this postcentral area is concerned with the finer and more conscious interpretations of the sensations of pressure, temperature, and muscular conditions, and especially the higher type of these sensations which we can project or localize accurately. The definite part of the cortex, if any, Cen/rai the pyramidal system of fibers. The lemniscus filters may be traced forward (see Fig. !)()) as far as the superior colliculus of the corpora quadrigemina and the thalamus, the important termination being in the thalamus (ventral or lateral nucleus). Those neurons that end in the thalamus are continued forward by a third set of neurons, which end in t he parietal lobe of the cerebrum (see big. si , < '). On its way through the medulla and pons f he lemniscus is believed to receive acce ion- of Bensory fibers from the sensory nuclei of the cranial nerves of the opposite side. The course of the lem- niscue hae been traced by various means, but especially by the SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 203 method of myelinization during embryonic life and by degenera- tion consequent upon long-standing disuse. As was stated in the section upon Nerve Degeneration, injury to an axon is followed quickly by degeneration of the peripheral end, and much more slowly by a degeneration of the central end and the nerve cell itself, when the path is not again established. Certain long-standing cystic lesions (porencephaly) in the parietal cor- tex have resulted in an atrophic degeneration of the lemniscus fibers, thus adding materially to the evidence that this sensory tract ends eventually in the region indicated.* Further evidence of the same character is found in the observations made by Campbellf upon cases of tabes dorsalis. The lesion in such cases is in the posterior funiculi of the spinal cofd, but eventually the whole upward path is affected and degenerative changes are found in the cells of the posterior central convolution. From the connections of the lemniscus with the tracts of the posterior funiculi of the cord it is evident that it forms one pathway at least for the fibers of muscle sense. Whether or not the fibers of pressure, pain, and temperature take the same route is not known, but it seems probable, at least, from the known connections of the lemniscus with the sensory nuclei of the cranial nerves and with the sensory tracts of the lateral as well as the posterior funiculi of the cord. The lemniscus ends chiefly in the thalamus, before passing on to the cortex, and here, as in other similar cases, we have the possibility that the lower centers, in addition to the reflex connections which they make, may mediate also some form of conscious reaction. While the general tendency has been to confine the conscious quality of the central reactions to the cortex, there is no proof that the lower centers are entirely lacking in this property. In Goltz's dog without cerebral cortex, for instance, the animal responded to various sensoiy stimuli, and when hungry gave evidence, so far as his actions were concerned, of experiencing the sensations of hunger; but whether or not these actions were associated with conscious sensations is hidden from us, and we can hope to arrive at positive conclusions upon this point only by obser- vations upon man himself. The Center for Vision. — The location in the cortex of the general area for vision has been established by anatomical, physio- logical, and clinical evidence. The physiologists have experimented chiefly by the method of ablation. Munk, Ferrier, and later ob- servers have found that removal of both occipital lobes is followed by defects in vision. According to Munk, removal of both occip- * Hosel, 'Archiv f. Psychiatrie, " 24, 452, 1892. t Campbell, ' Histological Studies on Localisation of Cerebral Functions, " Cambridge, 1905. 204 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. ital lobes is followed by complete loss of visual sensations, or, as he expresses it, by cortical blindness. Goltz, however, contends that in the dog at least removal of the entire cerebral cortex leaves the animal with some degree of vision, since he will close his eyes if a strong light is thrown upon them. All the experiments upon the higher mammals (monkeys) and clinical experience upon man tend, however, to support the view of Munk. Complete removal of the occipital lobes is followed by apparently total blindness. If any degree of vision remains it is not sufficient for recogni- tion of familiar objects or for directing the movements. In an animal in this condition the pupil is constricted when light is thrown upon the eye; but this reaction we may regard as a reflex through the midbrain, and there is no reason to believe that it is accompanied by a visual sensation. When the injury to the occip- ital cortex is unilateral the blindness affects symmetrical halves of the two eyes, a condition known as hemiopia. Destruction of the right occipital lobe causes blindness in the two right halves of the eyes, or, in accordance with the law of projection of retinal stimuli, in the two left halves of the normal visual field when the eyes are fixed upon any object. Destruction of the left occipital lobe is followed by blindness in the two left halves of the retinas or the right halves of the visual field. This result of physiological ex- periments is borne out by clinical experience. Any unilateral injury to the occipital lobes is followed by a condition of hemiopia more or less complete according to the extent of the lesion. Obser- vation, however, has shown that this general symmetrical relation has one interesting and peculiar exception. The most important part of the retina in vision is the region of the fovea centralis, whose projection into the visual field constitutes the field of direct or central vision. It is said thai the hemiopia from unilateral lesions of the cortex does not involve this pari of the retina. The Histological Evidence. — The histological results supple- ment in a very satisfactory way the findings from physiology and pathology. The retina itself, considered from an embryologies! standpoint, i- an outgrowth from the brain vesicles, and is there- fore an outlying portion of the central nervous system. The optic fibers, in terms of the neuron doctrine, must be considered as axons of the nerve cells in the retina. If, therefore, an eye is enu- cleated or an optic nerve is cut the libers connected with the brain undergo secondary degeneration and their course; can be traced microscopically to the brain. By this means it lias been shown thai in man and 'lie mammalia there is a partial decus- sation of the optic fibers in the chiasma. The libers from the Loner side <>f each retina cross at this point to the opposite optic tract; those from the outer side of the retina do not decussate, SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 205 but pass into the optic tract of the same side. The fibers of the optic tract end mainly in the gray matter of the lateral genicu- late body, but some pass also to the thalamus (pulvinar) and some to the superior colliculus of the corpora quaclrigemina. These locations, therefore, particularly the lateral geniculates, must be considered as the primary optic centers. From these points the path is continued toward the cortex by new neurons whose axons constitute a special bundle, the occipitothalamic Occipital lobe. Occipito-thalamic radiation. Superior colliculus. Lateral geniculate. Thalamus. v— Retina. Fig. 91. — Diagram to indicate the general course of the fibers of the optic nerves and the bilateral connection between cortex and retina. radiation, lying in the occipital part of the internal capsule (see Fig. 81, D). A schema representing this course of the optic fibers is given in the accompanying diagram (Fig. 91). According to this schema, the general relations of each occipital lobe to the retinas of the two eyes is such that the right occip- ital cortex represents the cortical center for the two right halves of the retinas, while the left occipital lobe is the center for the two left halves of each retina, — a relation that agrees completely with the results of experimental physiology and clinical studies. 206 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. In addition to the fibers described, which may be regarded as the visual fibers proper, there are other fibers in the optic tracts and optic nerves whose physiological value is not entirely clear. The fillers of this kind that have been described are: (1) Inferior or Gudden's commissure. Fibers that pass from one optic tract to the other along the posterior border of the chiasma. These fibers form a commissural band connecting the two internal (or median) geniculate bodies, and possibly also the inferior colliculi. It seems probable that they belong to the central auditory path rather than to the visual system. (2) Fibers passing from the chiasma into the floor of the third ventricle. The further course of these fibers is not clearly known, but it is possible that they make connections with the nuclei of the third nerve. They will be referred to in the section on Vision in con- nection with the light reflex of the iris. (3) A superior com- missure. Several observers have claimed that there is a com- missural band along the anterior margin of the chiasma which connects one optic nerve or retina with the other. There are many points in connection with the course of the optic fibers and the physiology of the different parts of the occip- ital cortex which are unknown and require further investigation. Some of these points may be referred to briefly. The Amount of Decussation in the Chiasma. — According to the schema given above, half of the fibers in each optic nerve decussate in the chiasma. There is, however, no positive proof that the division of the fibers is so symmetrically made. In the lower vertebrates, — fishes, amphibia, reptiles, and most birds — the crossing is said to be complete, while in the mammalia a certain proportion of the fibers remain in the optic tract of the same side. In a general way, it would appear that the higher the animal is in t Ik- scale of development the larger is the number of fibers that do not cross in the chiasma. At least it is true that a larger num- ber remain uncrossed in man than in any of the mammalia, and if is also possible "r probable that the extent of decussation in man shows individual differences. There seems to be no acceptable suggestion regarding (lie physiological value of (his partial decus- sation other than i hat, of a probable relation to binocular vision. It h.i- been used to explain the physiological fact that simultaneous stimulation of symmetrical points in the two retinas gives us a single visual sen :it ion. The Projection or Localization of the Retina on the Occipital Cortex. — It would seem most probable that the paths from each -pot iii the retina terminate in a definite region of the occipital cortex, ami attempl have been made by various methods o, determine this relation. According to Henschen,* the * Hen chen; "Brain," L893. 170. SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 207 visual paths in man end around the calcarine fissure on the mesial surface of the brain, and this portion of the occipital lobe should be regarded as the true cortical center for vision, the re- mainder of the occipital cortex being perhaps the seat of visual memories or associations. There seems to be much evidence indeed that the immediate ending of the optic paths lies in this region. Thus, Donaldson* found, upon examination of the brain of Laura Bridgman, — the blind deaf-mute, that the cuneus espe- cially showed marked atrophy, and clinical cases of lesions of the cuneus have been found to be associated with a marked degree of hemiopia. So also Flechsig,f by means of the myelinization method, finds that the optic fibers end chiefly along the margin of the calcarine fissure. It has been assumed that the fibers from the fovea of the retina t . ,, . . ,. LEFT RETINA end in this region, — according to some authors (Henschen) along the anterior third of the fissure, according to others (Schmid and LaqueurJ) along the posterior portion of the fissure. Moreover, since uni- lateral lesions of the occipital lobe, however extensive, do not cause complete blindness of the foveal region, it has been supposed that this im- portant part of the retina is bilaterally represented in the cortex, as indicated in the ac- companying diagram (Fig. 92), so that complete foveal blind- ness—that is, blindness of the centers of the visual fields — can only occur when both occipital lobes are injured in the region of the calcarine fissure. "While the general opinion seems to be that this last-named region is the main cortical ending of the retinal fibers, especially of those arising from the foveal area, other observers contend that the entire occipital cortex, lateral as well as mesial surfaces, must be regarded as the cortical termination of the visual paths, and that even the foveal portion of the retina is con- * Donaldson, "American Journal of Psychology," 1S92, 4. t Flechsig, "Localization der geistigen Vorgange," Leipzig, 1896. j Schmid and Laqueur, "Virchow's Archiv," 158, 1900. cC;piT'xU Fig. 92. — Diagram showing the probable relations between the parts of the retina and the visual area of the cortex. — (From Schd- fer.) The bilateral representation of the fovea is Indicated by the course of the dotted lines. Jlls PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. nected with a wide area in this lobe. Those who hold this view explain the known fact that lesions in the region of the calcarine fissure give the most permanent condition of hemiopia, on the view that these lesions involve the underlying fibers of the occipitothalamic radiation. Monakow,* for instance, points out that while extensive lesions of the occipital cortex on both sides leave, with a few exceptions, some degree of central vision, no cases are reported of cortical lesions involving only or mainly the vision in the macular region. He, therefore, argues that while the paths from the retina to the lower visual centers (lateral geniculate) may be isolated, the further connections with the cortex must be widespread. The cortical center for distinct vision according to this view is not limited to a narrow area, but must involve a large region in the occipital cortex. It is difficult to reconcile this view with the ideas of isolated conduction and specific function of each part of the cortex, and it is very evident that the projection of the retinas upon the cortex is a question that must be left for further observation and experiment. Some light was supposed to be thrown upon the subject from the results of stimulation of the occipital cortex. Stimulation of this kind causes movements of the eyes, and the movements vary with the place stimulated, f Stimulation of the upper border of the lobe causes movements of the eyes downward, stimulation of the lower border movements upward, and of intermediate regions, movements to the side. Assuming that the direction of the movement is toward that part of the visual field from which a normal visual stimulus would come, it is evident that movements of the eyes downward would imply stimulation of the upper half of the retina, since objects in the lower part of the visual field form their image on the upper half of the retina. Following this suggestion, the projection of the retinas on the occipital lobes or the cortical representation of the retinas on the occipital cortex might be represented by a definite schema. A definite relationship of this kind is indicated to some extent by clinical facts, that is to say, lesions of the lower region of the occipital lobes cause blindness chiefly in the lower portion of the retina and lesions of the anterior region in the upper portion of the retina. The fact that stimulation of the occipital cortex causes definite movements of the eyeballs seems to imply that there are efferent fibers in the occipitothal- amic radiation running from t he occipital cortex to the midbrain, where they make connect ions wit h 1 he motor nuclei of the third, fourth, and sixth cranial nerves. * Monakow, loc. cit., also "Ergebnisse d. Physiologic, " MJ07. fSchafer, "Brain," ll, I, L889, and 13, 165, L890. SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 209 The Function of the Lower Visual Centers. — The first ending of the optic fibers lies in the lateral geniculate and to a lesser extent in the thalamus and superior colliculus. It is conceiv- able, of course, that some degree of visual sensation may be mediated through these centers. Goltz observed that in dogs with the cerebrum removed the animals showed a constriction of the pupils when a bright light was thrown upon the eyes or even closed the eyes. It is the general belief that reactions of this kind are mechanical reflexes accompanied by no higher psychical reaction than in the case of spinal reflexes. The existence in the midbrain of the motor nuclei of the third nerve, and of the medial longitudinal fasciculus through which con- nections are established with the motor nuclei of other cranial nerves, furnishes us with a possible reflex arc through which the visual impulses brought into the lower optic centers, especially the superior colliculus, may cause co-ordinated movements of the eyes or of the head. Usually it is assumed that conscious visual sensations, and especially visual associations and mem- ories, are aroused only after the impulses reach the occipital cortex. In the fishes the midbrain forms the final ending of the optic fibers, and in these animals, therefore, whatever psychical activity accompanies the visual processes must be mediated through this portion of the brain. In the higher animals, how- ever, the development of a cerebral cortex is followed by the evolution of the occipitothalamic radiation, and as the connec- tions of the occipital cortex increase in importance, those of the midbrain (with the optic fibers) dwindle correspondingly. Here, as in other cases, the psychical activity is concentrated in the por- tions of the brain lying most anteriorly, and doubtless the degree of consciousness is greatly intensified in the higher animals in cor- respondence with the development of the cerebral cortex, whose striking characteristic is its capacity to evoke a psychical reaction. The Auditory Center. — The location of the auditory area has been investigated along lines similar to those used for the visual center. The experimental physiological work has yielded varying results in the hands of different observers. Munk and Ferrier placed the cortical center for hearing in the temporal lobe, and in spite of negative results by Schafer and others this localization has been shown to be substantially correct. Entire ablation of both temporal lobes is followed by complete deafness. Ablation on one side, however, is followed only by impairment of hearing, and in the light of the results from histology and from the clinical side it seems probable that the connections of the auditory cortex with the ear follow the general schema of the optical system rather than that of the body senses. That is, it is probable that the 14 210 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. auditory fibers from each ear end partly on the same side and partly or mainly on the opposite side of the cerebrum. The exact portion of the temporal lobe that serves as the immediate organ of auditory sensations cannot be determined with certainty, but it seems probable that it lies mainly in the superior temporal gyrus and the transverse gyri extending from this into the lateral fissure of the cerebrum (fissure of Sylvius). The Histological Evidences. — On the histological side the paths of the auditory fibers have been followed with a large measure of Posterior nucleus. Deiters's nucleus. Dorsal nucleus. Ventral nucleus. Cochlear branch. Vestibular branch. Semicircular canals. Scarpa's ganglion. Cochlea. Spiral ganglion. Mi'- medullary nuclei of the eighth nerve. (From Poireer and Charpy.) Buccess, although in many details the. opinions of the different tigators vary considerably. The eighth cranial nerve springe from the bulb by two roots: the external and the internal. The former has been shown to supply, mainly at least, the cochlear portion of the internal ear, and is, there- lore, the auditory tierve proper. This division is spoken of ae the cochlear branch. The internal root supplies mainly the vestibular branch of the internal ear, and is, therefore, spoken of :> the vestibular branch (see Fig. 93). If seems cer- tain that the latter i no1 an auditory nerve, but is concerned SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 211 with peculiar sensations from the semicircular canals and vestibule that have an important influence on muscular activity, especially in complex movements. The central course of these two roots is quite as distinct as their peripheral distribution, — a fact that bears out the supposition that they mediate different functions. The vestibular branch ends in the nucleus of Deiters, the nucleus of Bechterew, and the nucleus fastigii of the cerebellum. Through these nuclei reflex connections are made with the motor centers of the cord and midbrain, and probably also with the cerebellum. The path is not known to be continued lorward to the cerebrum. The central course of the cochlear branch is indicated schematically in Figs. 93 and 94. The fibers constituting this branch arise from nerve cells in the modiolus of the cochlea, — the spiral ganglion. These cells, like those in the poste- rior root ganglia, are bi- polar. One axon passes peripherally to end around the sense cells of the cochlea, at which point the sound waves arouse the nerve im- pulses. The other axon passes toward the pons, forming one of the fibers of the cochlear branch. On entering the pons these cochlear branches end in two nuclei, one lying ventral to the res- tiform body and known as the ventral or acces- sory nuclei (V.n., Fig. 94), and one dorsally, known as the dorsal nucleus or the tuber- culum acusticum (D.n.). From these nuclei the path is continued by secondary sensory neurons, and its further course toward the brain is still a matter of much uncertainty in regard to many of the details.* The general course of the fibers, however, is known Those axons that arise from the accessory nucleus pass mainly to the opposite side by slightly different routes (Fig. 94). * For literature, see Van Gehuchten, "Le Nevraxe, " 4, 253, 1903, and 8, 127, 1906. Fig. 94. — Diagram to show central course of auditory fibers (modified from Van Gehuchten): D.n., Dorsal nucleus giving rile to the fibers that form the medullary striae (a.s.); V.n., the ventral nucleus, giving origin to the fibers of the corpus trapezoideum {c.tr.); s.o., superior olivary nucleus; l.f., lateral lemniscus; n.s., nucleus of the lateral lemniscus; t.g.i.,, the inferior colliculus. 212 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. ►Some strike directly across toward the ventral side of the pons, forming a conspicuous band of transverse fibers that has long been known as the corpus trapezoideum; others pass dorsally around the restiform body and then course downward through the tegmental region to enter the corpus trapezoideum. The fibers of this cross hand end, according to some observers, in certain nuclei of gray matter on the opposite side of the pons, especially in the superior olivary body and the trapezoidal nucleus, and thence the path forward is continued by a third neuron. Certainly from the level of the superior olivary body the auditory fibers enter a distinct tract long known to the anat- omist and designated as the lateral fillet or lateral lemniscus. Authors differ as to whether the auditory fibers of this tract arise from nerve cells in the superior olivary and neighboring nuclei, or are the fibers from the accessory nucleus which pass by the superior olivary body without ending and then bend to run for- ward in a longitudinal direction. This last view is represented in the schema (Fig. 94). The secondary sensory fibers that arise in the tuberculum acusticum pass dorsally and then transversely, forming a band of fibers that comes so near to the surface of the floor of the fourth ventricle as to form a structure visible to the eye and known as the medullary or auditory stria?. The fibers of this system dip inward at the raphe, cross the mid-line, and a part of them at least eventually reach the lateral lemniscus of the other side either with or without ending first around the cells of the superior olivary nucleus. According to the descriptioD of some authors, the fibers from the accessory nucleus and tuberculum acusticum do not all cross the mid-line to reach the lateral lemniscus of the other side; some of them pass into the lateral lemniscus of the same side; so that the relations of the fibers of the cochlear nerves to the lateral lemnis- cus resemble, in the matter of crossing, the relations of the optic fiber- to the optic tract. After entering the lateral lemniscus the auditory fibers puss forward toward the midbrain and end in part in the gray matter of the inferior colliculus of the median or internal geniculate, and, according to Van Gehuchten, in a small mass of nerve cells in the midbrain known as the superior nucleus of the lemniscus. From this second or third termination another set of fibers, the auditory r;i,< I ial ion, continues forward through the inferior extremity of the internal capsule to end in the mperior temporal gyrus (see Fig. si, E). According to Plechsig,* who has studied the course of these fibers in the embryo by the myelinization method, the main group passes from the median geniculates to the transverse gyri of the tern- * Flech ig, ' Localisation der geistigen Vorgange," Leipzig, L896. SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 213 poral lobe within the lateral fissure of the cerebrum (fissure of Sylvius). The median geniculates, in man at least, have, therefore, the function of a subordinate auditory center, as the lateral geniculates have the function of a subordinate visual center. The median geniculates are connected with the inferior colliculus, and also, it will be remembered, with each other, by commissural fibers (Gudden's commissure) that pass along the optic tracts and the inferior margin of the chiasma. The auditory path, therefore, involves the following structures: The spiral ganglion, the cochlear nerve, accessory nucleus and tuberculum acusticum, corpus trapezoideum, medullary striae, superior olivary, lateral lemniscus, inferior colliculus, median geniculate, Gudden's commissure, auditory radiation, and temporal cortex. The Motor Responses from the Auditory Cortex. — According to Ferrier, stimulation of the cortex of the temporal lobe (inferior convolution) causes definite movements, such as pricking of the ears and turning of the head and eyes to the opposite side. As in the case of the visual area, therefore, we must suppose that distinct motor paths originate in the auditory region, and it is natural to suppose that these paths give a means for cortical reflex movements following upon auditory stimulation. The Olfactory Center. — The olfactory sense is quite un- equally developed in different mammals. Broca divided them from this standpoint into two classes: the osmatic and the anosmatic group, the latter including the cetacea (whales, porpoise, dolphin) . The osmatic group in turn has been divided into the microsmatic and macrosmatic animals, the latter class including those animals in which the sense of smell is highly developed, such as the dog and rabbit, while the former includes those animals, such as man, in which this sense is relatively rudimentary.* The peripheral end- organ of smell consists of the olfactory epithelium in the upper portion of the nasal chambers. The physiolog}^ of this organ will be considered in the section on special senses. The epithelial cells of which it consists are comparable to bipolar ganglion cells. The processes or hairs that project into the nasal chamber are acted upon by the olfactory stimuli, and the impulses thus aroused are conveyed by the basal processes of the cells, the olfac- tory fibers, through the cribriform plate of the ethmoid bone into the olfactory bulb. The Olfactory Bulb and its Connections.— The olfactory bulbs are outgrowths from and portions of the cerebral hemi- spheres. Each bulb is connected with the cerebral hemispheres by its olfactory tract. The connections established by the fibers * See Barker, "The Nervous System," 1899, for references to literature. 214 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. of this tract are widespread, complicated, and in part incom- pletely known. All those portions of the brain connected with t he sense of smell are sometimes grouped together as the rhinenceph- alon. According to von Kolliker, the parts included under this designation are, in addition to the olfactory bulb and tract, Am- nion's horn, the fascia dentata, the hippocampal lobe, the fornix, the septum pellucidum, and the anterior commissure. The schematic connections of the olfactory fibers are as follows (Fig. 95): After ■ -Diagram of the central course of the olfactory fibers: /, Olfactory bulb; //, olf < , III, cortex of the hippocampal lobe (gyrus uncinatus): / V, anterioi Commit lire, olfactory portion; A, olfactory epithelial cells of nose (their fibers, olfactory nerve libers, terminate in the glomeruli of the bulb)] It, glomeruli 01 olf actory bulb where the olfactory fibers come in contact with the dendrites ot the mitral cells; c, mitral and bru-h cells; I, -', 3, axons from the mitral cells constituting the fibers of the olfactory tract. Fibei •!, which enter the commissure, arise, according to somo observers, from cells in the olfactory li.ue near the base of the tract. entering the olfactory lobe the libers terminate in certain globular bodies, the glomeruli olfactorii (B), whose diameter varies from 0.1 to 0.3mm. Here connections are made by contact with the dendrites of nerve cells of the olfactory lobe, the mitral and brush cells (Q. The axons of these cells pass toward the brain iii the olfactory had,. Three bundles of these fibers are distinguished: (1) The precommis- sural bundle, the fibers of which terminate in part in nerve cells sit- uated in the tract itself, but, for the most part, enter the anterior commit jure and pass to the same or the opposite side, to end in the hippocampal lobes or Other gray matter belonging to the rhinen- cephalon. (2) The mesial bundle, the fibers of which terminate SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 215 in the gray matter adjacent to the base of the olfactory tract, the tuberculum olfactorium, whence the path is probably continued by other neurons to the region of the hippocampal lobe. (3) The lateral tract, whose fibers seem' to pass to the hippocampal lobe of the same side. According to Van Gehuchten,* none of the fibers of the anterior commissure arise from the nerve cells in the olfactory bulb. He considers that the fibers in the olfactory portion of this commissure constitute an association system connecting the olfac- tory lobe of one side with the olfactory bulb of the other side. The Cortical Center for Smell. — So far as the histological evidence goes, it tends to show that the chief cortical termination of the olfactory paths is found in the hippocampal convolution, especially its distal portion, the uncus. The experimental evi- dence from the side of physiology points in the same direction. Ferrier states that electrical stimulation in this region is followed by a torsion of the lips and nostrils of the same side, muscular movements that accompany usually strong olfactory sensations. On the other hand, ablations of these regions are followed by de- fects in the sense of smell. The experimental evidence is not very satisfactory, owing to the technical difficulties in operating upon these portions of the brain without at the same time involving neighboring regions. There is some clinical evidence also that lesions in this region involve the sense of smell. Thus Carbonieri records that a tumor in this portion of the temporal lobe occa- sioned epileptic attacks which were accompanied by nauseating odors. The Cortical Center for Taste Sensations. — Practically nothing definite is known concerning the central paths and cortical termination of the taste fibers. The course of these fibers in the peripheral nerves has been much investigated and the facts are mentioned in the section upon " special senses." It is usually assumed, although without much decisive proof, that the cortical center lies also in the hippocampal convolution posterior to the area of olfaction. Experimental lesions in this region, according to Ferrier, are accompanied by disturbances of the sense of taste. On embryological grounds Flechsig supposes that the cortical center may lie in the posterior portion of the gyrus fornicatus (6, Fig. 98). Aphasia. — The term aphasia means literally the loss of the power of speech. It was used originally to indicate the condition of those who from accident or disease affecting the brain had lost in part or entirely the power of expressing themselves in spoken words, but the term as a general expression is now extended to include those who are unable to understand spoken or written language, — * Van Gehuchten, "Le Nevraxe," 6, 191, 1904. 216 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. that is, those who arc word-blind or word-deaf. It is usual, there- fore, to distinguish sensory aphasia from motor aphasia. By the latter term is meant the condition of those who arc unable to speak, and by sensory aphasia those who are. unable to understand the written, printed, or spoken symbols of words. Motor Aphasia. — A condition of motor aphasia not infrequently results from injuries to the head or from hemorrhage in the region of the middle cerebral artery. The first exact knowledge of the portion of the brain involved seems to have been obtained by Bouillaud (1S25) as the result of numerous autopsies. (It is a curious fact that Bouillaud's observations were inspired by the work of Gall. Gall having observed, as he thought, that individuals who are fluent speakers or who have retentive memories are characterized by projecting eyes, concluded that this peculiarity is due to the larger size of the lower part of the frontal lobe, and he therefore located the faculty of speech in this region of the brain. In spite of the vagaries into which he was led by his false methods Gall made many most important contributions to our knowledge of the anatomy of the brain and the cord. The discovery of the location of the center of speech, however, cannot be rightly placed to his credit, since his reasons for its location were, so far as we know, entirely unjustified. It cannot be reckoned as more than a coincidence that in this particular his phrenological localization was afterward in a measure justified by facts.) The essential truth of Bouillaud's observations was established by other observers, and Broca especially located the part of the brain involved in t hese lesions in the posterior part of the third or inferior frontal convolution. This region is, therefore, frequently known as Broca's convolution or Broca's center. Subsequent ob- servations have in general confirmed this localization, and what is designated as the "speech center" has been placed in the inferior frontal convolution in the gyrus surrounding the anterior or ascending limb of the lateral fissure (fissure of Sylvius, »S, Fig. 90). Many authors insist that this localization is too limited, and thai defects in the power of speech may result not only from injuries to this region but also from lesions of con- tiguous areas, including the anterior portion of the island and the opercular portion of the central convolution. Autopsies have shown thai in right-handed persons the speech center is placed or IS functional usually in the left cerebral hemisphere, while ;n the case of left-handed individuals aphasia and paralysis are produced by lesions involving the right side of the brain. This region is not I he dired corl ical motor center for the muscles of speech. It is possible that aphasia may exist without paralysis of the e latter muscles. It, is rather the memory center of the motor innervations necessary to form the appropriate sounds or words wit h which we have learned to express certain concepts. The child is taught to express certain ideas by definite words, and the memory apparatus through which these associations SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 217 are transmitted to the motor apparatus may be conceived as located in the speech center. Lesions of any kind affecting this area will, therefore, destroy more or less the ability to use appropriately spoken words, and clinical experience shows that motor aphasia may be exhibited in all degrees of completeness and in many curious varieties. The individual may retain the power to use a limited number of words, with which he expresses his whole range of ideas, as, for instance, in the case described by Broca,* in which the individual retained for the expression of numbers only the word ' three," and was obliged to make this word do duty for all numerical concepts. Other cases are recorded in which the patient had lost only the power to use names — that is, nouns ("Marie") — or could remember only the initial letters. Others still in which words could be used only when associated with musical memories, as in singing. Motor aphasia is classified sometimes as complete and incomplete. In the former case there is entire loss of the power of expression, including eventually the power to write or to use symbols. The incomplete form includes a number of varieties and is sometimes further divided into the subcortical and the transcortical, accord- ing to the supposed location of the lesion. When the individual can conceive the words, but is unable to express them in spoken language or expresses them incompletely, the lesion is supposed to be subcortical. When, on the contrary, there is inability to conceive the words, an interference with the inner word-building, the lesion is supposed to affect the cortex. It does not seem to be certain whether or not, in the case of complete lesion of the center on one side, the ability to speak can be again acquired by education of new centers, f Some recorded cases seem to indicate that this re-education is possible in the young, while in the old it is more difficult or impossible. We express our thoughts not only in spoken but also in written symbols. As this latter form of expression involves a different set of muscles and a different educational experience, it is natural to assume that the complex associations concerned or, to use a convenient expression, the memory centers should involve a different part of the cortex. It is, in fact, observed that in some aphasics the loss of the power of writing, a condition desig- nated as agraphia, is the characteristic defect, rather than the loss of the ability to use articulate language. The area in which the motor associations for the act of writing are located has been placed in the middle or second frontal convolution con- * Exner, "Hermann's Handbuch der Physiologie, " vol. iii, part n, p. 342. Consult for older literature. f See Mills, " Journal of the Amer. Med. Assoc," 1904, xliii. 218 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. tiguous to the cortical motor centers for the muscles of the arm and hand (11". Fig. 96). Sensory Aphasia. — In sensory aphasia* the individual suffers from an inability to understand spoken or written language, ami as the cortical centers for hearing and seeing are situated in distinct parts of the brain, we should expect that the mech- anism for the association, in one case of visual memories of verbal symbols with certain concepts and in the other case of auditory memories, should also be located in separate regions. Inability to under- stand spoken lan- guage, or word-deaf- ness, is, in fact, usually attributed to a lesion involving the supe- rior temporal convo- lution contiguous to the cortical sense of hearing (H, Fig. 96), while loss of power to understand written or printed language, word - blindness, is traced to lesions in- volving the inferior parietal convolution, the gyrus angularis, contiguous to the oc- cipital visual center (V, Fig. 96). These two conditions may occur together, bu1 cases are recorded in which t hey existed independently. 1 1 may be im- agined thai the individual suffering from word-blindness alone is r — »-m tally in i lie comlii ion of one who attempts to read a foreign language. The power of vision exists, but the verbal symbols have no associal ions, I berefore, no meaning. So one who is word- deaf alone may be compared to I he normal individual who is Bpoken to in a foreign tongue. The words are hea.nl, but they ociations with past experience. Sensory aphasia, I il.f motor aphasia, may be complete or incomplete. In the com- plete form there is \vord-de;d'ness as well as word-blindness, but the individual may be capable of speaking. In the incomplete type these symptoms are exhibited in milder form. The exact 1 on 'lit Starr, "Aphasia," " Transactions of the Congress of American Physicians and Burgeons," vol. i, p. :»'j!», L888; also Monakow, "Gehirn* pathologie," L906. H Fig. 90. — Lateral view of a human hemisphere; cortical area V, damage to which produces 'mind- blindness" (word-blindness): cortical area //, damage to which produces "mind-deafness" (word-deafness); cortical area n anus. The location of these areas is shown in part in Figs. 97 and 98, 18 (0b, if), '■ >;. 7 (7,J), .9, IB. TWO areas connected with the olfactory sense are not shown in these figures; they appear in the anterior per- forate lamina on the base of the brain and in the uncinate gyrus. Brain," 1903, p. 215. t Rech ij.r, "Berichte der mathematisch-physischen Klasse der konigl, = 0 der Wissenschaften zu Leipzig," 1904. For a summary of 1 1,,, results of this work, see Sabin, " The Johns Hopkins Hospital Bulletin," l ebruary, 1906. SENSE AREAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 223 "-""''"Ir f - ■*:>:« £x&i!'r Fig. 97. — Lateral surface of the brain, showing the primordial areas, both sensory and automatic, in dotted zones. — (Flechsig.) ft ■ '-:;-^v-h^vvA^-'' 1S* $ .9 '■*. I Fig. 98. — Same zones on the mesial surface of the brain. — (Flechsig). 224 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. • •:;.- v-;/;'-Vr'A-::";;;' -. Vv«-^v: };,,.. \ >,/• .PJ 99. — Lateral surfaces of the brain, showing the primordial and marginal zones. — (Flechsig.) I :ir, 33 '/"■ 23 tp 36 i ig 100. Same area* . Primary areas without projection fibers {3, 9, 10, 11, 12, 13) and apparently without association fibers. Functions un- certain. II. Association areas. IIa* Intermediate or border areas, 14, 16-33, provided with short association fibers. II Terminal or central areas, 34, 35, 36, provided with long association fibers. Histological Differentiation in Cortical Structure. — While the general structure of the cortex is everywhere similar, detailed examination has shown differences in the shape of the cells, the thickness and number of the strata, the calibre of the fibers, etc., which are said to be constant for any given region. By this means it is possible to divide the cerebral cortex into a number of areas whose structures are sufficiently distinct to be recognized with some certainty. Reasoning from analogy we should infer that a differen- tiation in structure implies a subdivision of physiological activity, and to this extent this recent histological work supports the view of a localized distribution of function in the cortex. Campbell,* * Campbell, "Histological Studies on Localisation of Cerebral Functions," Cambridge, 1905. 15 226 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. in a very thorough investigation of this kind, has succeeded in separating some fifteen or sixteen different areas, and the results obtained by him support in a general way the localizations described in the preceding pages. Thus the cortex in the postcentral con- volution (body-sense area) has a structure distinctly different from that of the precentral convolution (motor area), the latter being characterized among other things by the presence of giant pyramidal cells (Betz cells). In the occipital lobes the region round the calcarine fissure (visuosensory ) has a structure different from that of the contiguous cortex (visuopsychic), and a similar difference is claimed for the auditor}- region. Campbell calls attention to the fact that the extreme end of the frontal lobe (prefrontal region) has a comparatively undeveloped structure. Fig. 101. Diagram to show the composition of the corpus callosum aa a system of com- missural fibers, without projection fibers. — (Cajal.) The Corpus Callosum. — The corpus callosum is the must conspicuous of the bands of commissural fibers that connect one cerebral hemisphere with the other. Similar tracts of the same general nature are the anterior commissure and the fornix. The position and great development of the corpus callosum has made it I lie ol.jert of e\ periinenl al as well as anatomical invest igat ion. When t he corpus is divided by a section along t he Longitudinal fissure (v. Koranyi) no perceptible effect of either a motor or sensory nature is observed in the animal. When it is stimulated electrically (Mott andSchafer) from above, symmetri cal movements on the two sides of the body may be obtained. If the motor cortex on one side is removed, stimulation in the lon- gitudinal fissure causes movements only on the side controlled by the uninjured cortex. These facts are in harmony with the SENSE AKEAS AND ASSOCIATION AREAS. 227 results of histological studies, which indicate that the fibers of the corpus callosum do not enter directly into the internal capsules to be distributed to underlying portions of the brain, but are truly commissural and connect portions of the cortex of one hemisphere with the cortex of the other side. This relation is indicated in the accompanying diagram (Fig, 101). So far as the motor regions are concerned, there is some evidence that the connection thus es- tablished is between symmetrical parts of the cortex (Muratoff), — that is, between parts having similar functions, — and we may per- haps regard the corpus as a means by which the functional activities of the two sides of the cerebrum are associated. The Corpora Striata and Thalami. — The numerous masses of gray matter found in the cerebrum beneath the cortex, in the thalamencephalon, and in the midbrain have each, of course, specific functions, but, in general, it may be said that they are intercalated on the afferent or efferent paths to or from the cortex. Their physiology is included, therefore, in the description of the functions mediated by these paths. For instance, the lateral geniculate bodies form part of the optic path. In addition, however, these masses of cells contain in many cases reflex arcs of a more or less complicated kind, through which afferent impulses are converted into efferent impulses that affect the musculature or the glandular tissues of the body. The large nuclei constituting the corpora striata (nucleus caudatus and n. lenticularis) and the thalami have been frequently studied experimentally to ascertain whether they have specific functions independently of their rela- tions to the cortex. These efforts have given uncertain results. Older experiments (Nothnagel), in which the attempt was made to destroy these nuclei by the localized injection of chromic acid, are probably unreliable, as the destruction involved also the projection fibers passing to the cortex. Lesions of the nucleus caudatus are said to be accompanied always by a rise in body temperature and an increase in heat production, and stimulation of the same nucleus gives a very marked rise in blood-pressure. These facts indicate a possible connection of this nucleus with heat and vasomotor regulation. Other observers have supposed that these nuclei are especially concerned in the co-ordination of the muscles employed in involuntary or unconscious movements. On the anatomical side we have the striking fact that the nuclei of the corpora striata have few connections with the cortex, but, on the other hand, send an independent system of projection fibers into the brain stem. Embryologically these structures are developed from the wall of the forebrain and they would seem to have a physiological im- portance similar to that of the cortex itself, but experimental 228 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. and clinical facts are at present insufficient to justify any hypothesis as to their special functions. With regard to the various nuclei of the thalamus it is known that they form abun- dant connections with the sensory areas of the cortex cerebri, and from this standpoint they may be regarded as consisting of subcenters, with a probability, however, that reflexes may occur through them (subcortical reflexes) independently of the cortex. Numerous fibers have been traced from the thalamus to the body sense area (Flechsig). CHAPTER XI. THE FUNCTIONS OF THE CEREBELLUM, THE PONS, AND THE MEDULLA. The functions of the cerebellum are, in some respects, less satis- factorily known than those of any other part of the central nervous system. Many theories have been held. Most of these views have been attempts to assign to the organ a single function of a definite character, but latterly the insufficiency of the theories proposed has led observers to attribute to the cerebellum general properties the nature of which can not be expressed satisfactorily in a single phrase. Before attempting to give a summary of exist- ing views it will be helpful to recall briefly the important facts re- garding its structure and relations, so far as they are known and can be used to explain its functional value. Anatomical Structure and Relations of the Cerebellum. — The finer histology of the cerebellar cortex is represented in Fig. 102. Three layers may be distinguished. The external molecular layer (A), the middle granular layer (B), and the internal medullary- layer consisting of the white matter or medullated nerve fibers, afferent and efferent (C). Between the molecular and granular layers lie the large and characteristic Purkinje cells (a). The dendrites of these cells branch profusely in the molecular layer; their axons pass into the medullary layer. From the standpoint of the neuron doctrine these cells, so far as the cerebellum is con- cerned, are efferent. They form, indeed, the sole efferent system of the cerebellar cortex. The afferent fibers of the cerebellum end in both the granular and the molecular layers. Those that termi- nate in the granular layer — designated by Cajal as moss fibers, have at their terminations and points of branching curious clumps of small processes; they probably connect with the dendrites of the nerve cells in this layer. Those that pass deeper into the molec- ular layer come into connection with the dendrites of the Purkinje cells, around which, indeed, they seem to twine, so that Cajal desig- nated them as climbing fibers. The granular layer (B) contains numerous granules (g) or small nerve cells. These cells are spherical, and have a relatively large nucleus and a small amount of cyto- plasm. Their dendrites are few and short; their axons run into the molecular layer, divide in T, and the two branches then run 229 230 PHYSIOUKiY OF CENTRAL XKRYOUS SYSTEM. parallel to the surface and doubtless make connections with the den- drites of the Purkinje cells as well as with the cells of the molecular layer. A few larger nerve cells of Golgi's second type (/) are found also in the granular layer. In the molecular layer are found two types of cells: the larger basket cells (6) whose axons terminate in a group of small branches that inclose the body of the Purkinje cells, and a number of smaller cells (e), situated more superficially, whose axons pass longitudinally in the molecular layer and termi- nate in arborizations or baskets that doubtless make connections with the dendrites of the Purkinje cells. I ig. Mil'. Histology of (lie (••■icbclluin. (From ObcrBtviner.) A consideration of this peculiar and intricate structure enables us to comprehend that the cerebellar cortex presents a reflex arc of a very considerable degree of complexity. The incoming im- pulses through (lie moss and climbing fillers may pass at once to the Purkinje cells and lead to efferenl discharges, or they may end in the cells of the granular or molecular layer and thus be distributed to i lie Purkinje cells in a more indirect way. In addition to the cortex the cerebellum contains several masses of gray matter in it- interior: the large dentate nucleus in the center of each hemi- sphere and the group of nuclei lying in or near the middle of the CEREBELLUM, PONS, AND MEDULLA. 231 medullary substance of the vermiform lobe (nucleus fastigii, n. globosi, and the n. emboliformis). The axons of the Purkinje cells of the cortex terminate in these subcortical nuclei, and the efferent path from the cerebellum is then continued by new neurons. Thus, the fibers of the superior peduncles (brachium conjunctivum) of the cerebellum arise chiefly from the dentate nuclei, and only indirectly from the cortex. The anatomical connections, afferent and efferent, between the cerebellum and other parts of the nervous system are very complex and not yet entirely known. Without attempting to recall all of these connections, which will be found described in works upon anat- omy or neurology, emphasis may be laid upon those which are at present helpful in discussing the physiology of the organ. 1. Connections with the Afferent Paths of the Cord. — Through the inferior peduncles (restiform bodies) the cerebellum receives affer- ent fibers from the spinal cord and the medulla. The cerebello- spinal fasciculus undoubtedly terminates in the cerebellum, and according to some observers the fibers of the posterior funiculi after ending in the n. gracilis and n. cuneatus are also continued in part to the cerebellum by nerve fibers passing by way of the inferior peduncles. This latter view has, however, not found confirmation in recent work, most authors believing that the afferent fibers of the posterior funiculi all enter the lemniscus, after decussating, and pass forward to the thalamus. Ascending- fibers arising in the reticular formation of the medulla and the olivary nucleus also take this path to the cerebellum, and, on the other hand, probably make connections with the sensory tracts of the cord or the sensory nuclei of the medulla. Another afferent tract of the cord, that of Gowers (fasciculus anterolater- als superficialis), ends in the cerebellum, in large part at least, forming a part, in fact, of the cerebellospinal system. The nature of the sensory impulses conveyed in this way to the cerebellum is not entirely understood, but it seems certain that some of them, at least, are what we designate as impulses of deep sen- sibility, that is, sensibility of muscle, tendon, and joint, as opposed to cutaneous sensibility, and this fact, as we shall see, throws some light on the specific functional importance of the cerebellum. 2. Connections ivith the Vestibular Branch of the Eighth Cran- ial Nerve. — This branch, arising in the semicircular canals and utriculus and sacculus, ends in the pons in several nuclei (Deiters', Bechterew's) and also in the n. fastigii of the cerebellum. These nuclei, in turn, are connected with other parts of the central nervous system, but the details, are not yet completely known. The connections that have been most clearly established are those made with the motor centers. From Deiters' nucleus the 232 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL XLRYOUS SYSTEM. vestibulospinal tract arises and passes to the cord to connect with the motor centers of the spinal nerves, while Bechterew's nucleus is said to give origin to a bundle which runs with the medial longitudinal fasciculus and may make connections with the motor nuclei of the cranial nerves. In how far the vestibular L03 Diagram to indicate a possible descending path from cerebrum to cord in ad- dition i" ill'- pyramidal system, namely, tin- secondary or cerebellar motoi path (Van Gehuchten). The path i- indirect and comprises tbe following units: I. The cortico ponto cerebellar path, represented as arising in tin- motoi area <>i the cerebrum and passing down with 'in- pyramidal ■■ item to end in the pons, thence continued through the middle peduncles '" tbe cerebellar cortex <>i opposite Bide. 2. i In- path from the cerebellar cortex in the dentate nucleus. 3. The path from the dentate nucleus to the red nucleus passing iperior peduncles, brachium conjunctivum. I. The path from the red nucleui to 'in- motoi cells "i the ptnal cord rubro pinal tract i. nuclei may make afferenl connections with the cerebellum is undecided, lmt it seems probable thai such tracts exist, iii view of the fad thai destruction of the semicircular canal and severe lesions of the cerebellum cause motor disturbances that are strikingly similar. :;. Connections "Hli Other Sensory Nuclei, In addition to the CEREBELLUM, PONS, AND MEDULLA. 233 special sensory connections just described, it is stated by various neurologists that the sensory nuclei of the vagus, the trigeminal and the auditory nerves, send afferent paths into the cerebellum, and that similar paths extend from the primary end stations of the optic fibers.* 4. Connections with the Cortex of the Cerebrum. — The cerebellar cortex is connected with the cerebral cortex by the large system known as the cortico-ponto-cerebellar tract (see Fig. 81, A). The fibers of this tract arise in the motor area of the cerebrum or in the frontal cortex anterior to the motor area, descend in the internal capsule and cerebral peduncle, and end in the gray matter of the pons. Thence new axons continue the path across the mid-line and to the cerebellar cortex by way of the middle peduncle (brachium pontis). The tract would seem to convey efferent impulses from the cerebral cortex (motor region) of one side to the cerebellar cortex of the opposite side. A second possible connection with the cerebrum is made by way of the thalamus. Fibers arising in the dentate nucleus emerge by way of the brachium conjunctivum and connect with the red nucleus in the subthalamic region and perhaps also with the thalamus. The latter fibers may be continued forward to the cortex of the cerebrum and thus constitute an afferent path from cerebellum to cerebrum. Those fibers, on the contrary, which end in the red nucleus are brought into reflex connection with the motor bundle (rubrospinal tract), extending from the red nucleus to the motor centers in the spinal cord. Making use of the connec- tions described above, Van Gehuchten pictures an indirect motor path from the cortex of the cerebrum to the motor nerves by way of the cerebellum (see Fig. 103). The motor impulses descend by way of the cortico-ponto-cerebellar path to the cerebellar cortex, thence to the dentate nucleus, thence to the red nucleus, and then, by way of the rubrospinal tract, to the motor nuclei of the spinal nerves. Theories Concerning the Functions of the Cerebellum.— Modern views concerning the functions of the cerebellum may be classified under three general heads: First, those that consider it a general co-ordinating center or organ for the muscular movements and especially for those concerned in equilibrium and locomotion. This view, first proposed essentially by Flourens (1824), has been adopted by many, perhaps by most, writers since his time. The manner in which the organ serves to co-ordinate these movements has been explained in various ways. According to the older ob- servers, it was supposed so to arrange or group the various motor impulses that they reached the lower motor centers in the cord * See Edinger, "Brain," 29, 483, 1906. 'J.'U PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. in the necessary combination for co-ordinated contractions. Ac- cording to more recent observers, this synergetic action is exor- cised not directly on the motor side of the reflex but on the sensory side. The numerous sensory paths connected with the organ, especially those of the muscular sense, and those from the vestibular nerve, suggest the view that in the complex cortex of the cerebel- lum these afferent impulses act upon nervous combinations whose discharges in turn are conveyed to the motor centers in a definite and orderly sequence. Either point of view assumes that there are in the cerebellum certain distinct mechanisms — that is, combi- nations of neurons that are essentially reflex centers, and that in all of our more complex bodily movements these mechanisms intervene. The second general set of theories regarding the cere- bellum assumes that this organ is essentially the center or a center for the muscle sense. This view is connected usually with the name of Lussana,* but has been supported since in one sense or another by many observers, f It is, in fact, not essentially different per- haps from the second phase of the first group of theories. Those who have expressed their idea of the physiology- of the cerebellum by saying that it is a center of the muscle sense have, in recent times at least, recognized that this sense has a cortical center also in the cerebrum. The view can not assume, thcrefoie, a conscious muscle sense mediated by the cerebellum, but only that the muscle sense fibers have a cortical termination therein and that the cerebel- lar activity thus aroused is in some way necessary to the orderly adjustment of complex voluntary movements. According to an- other point of view, the cerebellum is a great augmenting organ for the neuromuscular system. Jt is added on, as it were, to the cere- brospinal motor system and serves not to co-ordinate the motor discharges, but to increase their strength or effectiveness. This general view, first proposed by Weir Mitchell (1869), has been sup- ported by buys, and especially, although with important modifica- tion-, by Luciani.} Some of the details of the work of the latter observer are given below. ExperimentalWorkUpon the Cerebellum. — Rolando, and par- ticularly l-'loureiis, gave (lie direction to modern experimentation in this subject. '| he latter observer made numerous observations, e pecially on pigeons, in regard to the effect of removing all or a part of i In' cerebellum, lie describes in detail the striking results of auch an operation. When all or a large, pari of the organ isre- * Lussana. Sec "Journal , 418, 1862. i Bee Lewandowsky, "Archiv f. Physiologic, " L903, L29. j Ini- the literature of the cerebellum, see Lueiani, " II cervelleto, '' Flor- 1891: German tran lation,"Das EOeinhirn," L893. Also Lueiani,' article I'm Kleinhirn," in Ergebnie e der Physiologie, " vol. iii, pari n, p. 269, L904, and van Rynberk, ibid., vii, <>■',:;, L908. CEREBELLUM, PONS, AND MEDULLA. 235 moved the animal shows a most distressing inability to stand or move. There seems to be no muscular paralysis, but, at first, a total lack of power to co-ordinate properly the contractions of the various muscles involved in maintaining equilibrium. The animal takes a most abnormal position, with the head retracted and twisted, and any attempt to move is followed by violent disorderly contractions that may result in a series of involuntary somersaults. The animal is totally unable to fly. When the injury to the cere- bellum is less the effect upon the movements is either too slight to be noticed or is shown in a greater or less uncertainty in its movements. When it attempts to walk, for instance, it exhibits a staggering, drunken gait, a condition designated as cerebellar ataxia. Similar operations on mammals give in general the same results. If the operation is unilateral, — that is, affects only one hemisphere, — the animal (dog) exhibits forced movements, such as a tendency to roll around the long axis of his body toward the injured side and subsequently movements in a circle toward the same side. In man there are several cases on record in which the organ was shown by autopsy to be largely or completely atro- phied, and numerous cases of tumors affecting the cerebellum have also been reported. In the latter group of cases there may be certain marked subjective symptoms, such as headache, and especially vertigo, and also a certain degree of ataxia or awkwardness and uncertainty of movement. So also in the cases of atrophy, in which probably the condition developed slowly through a number of years, a degree of ataxia was ex- hibited, especially when the movements were rapid and forced. In the ataxic condition resulting from tabetic lesions of the posterior funiculi the effect upon the movements is increased by covering up the eyes (Romberg's. symptom), the individual being then deprived of his visual stimuli as well as those coming by way of the muscular and cutaneous nerves. In cerebellar ataxia, however, the effect is not increased by closure of the eyes, a result which is probably explained by the fact that the individual still possesses his paths of muscular and cutaneous sensibility to the cerebrum, and these senses may be used in the reflex adjustments of voluntary movements. Interpretation of the Experimental and Clinical Results. — Flourens was led by the striking results of his operations on pigeons to suggest the view that the cerebellum is an organ for the co- ordination of the movements of equilibrium and locomotion. Objections were raised to this view. Some observers (Dalton, Weir Mitchell) found that if the pigeons from which the cerebellum had been removed were kept long enough the effects first observed gradually disappeared, so that finally the animals were able to 236 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. move or fly with no marked difference from the normal animal except that fatigue was shown much more quickly. Hence the view advocated by Mitchell that the essential function of the cerebellum is that of an augmenting apparatus for the voluntary movements. With regard to this view it may be remarked in passing that pigeons with the cerebral hemispheres removed exhibit apparently as a permanent symptom the same tendency to rapid fatigue after sustained muscular effort. By the same logical process therefore one might conclude that one function of the cerebrum is that of an augmenting organ to the motor discharges from the cerebellum or midbrain. So also the cases of complete or nearly complete atrophy of the cerebellum in human beings in which no evil result followed other than a slight degree of cerebellar ataxia have been used as an argument against the view that this organ is necessary to the co-ordination of the complex voluntary move- ments. The view that the cerebellum has essentially a direct co-ordinating function has been criticized most seriously by Luciani. This observer made a series of long-continued and most careful observations upon dogs and monkeys in which the entire cere- bellum or certain definite parts had been removed. He lays stress upon the fact that the violent disturbance of movement is tem- porary and is slowly recovered from in time. He was led, therefore, to view these disturbances as due primarily not to the loss of the nor- mal functional activity of the organ, but to irritations resulting from the operation. When this stage of irritation is passed the real defects which indicate the true function of the cerebellum become apparent. These defects exhibit themselves as a loss of power in the neuromuscular apparatus of the complex voluntary move- ments, and he analyzes these results under three heads: First, a loss of force in the muscular contractions, — a condition of asthenia ; second, a loss of tone in the muscles of the limbs and trunk, par- ticularly in the hind limbs, — a condition of atonia; and, third, a loss of steadiness in the muscular contractions, — a condition of astasia. The astasia manifests itself in ;i tremor of the muscles when voluntarily contracted, especially in movements requiring much exertion. Luciani supposes that tliis tremor is due to an alteration thai is. ;i slowing of the rhythm of discharges of the impulses from the motor centers. The functions of the cerebellum on lii-- theory are expressed, therefore, by saying that if is an aug- menting organ for the activitj of the neuromuscular apparatus; and that, so far as this augmenting or si rengl hening activity can be ana- lyzed, it consists in .'in increase in the energy of (lie motor discharges (sthenic action), an increase in the tension or tone of the motor centers and their connected muscles (tonic action), ;md an increase in the rhythm of the motor impulses (static action) so that nor- CEREBELLUM, PONS, AND MEDULLA. 237 mally the muscular contractions are of the nature of complete tetani. Luciani believes that this action of the cerebellum is continuous, although varying in intensity, and that it affects all of the musculature of the body, and not simply the muscles con- cerned in body equilibrium. This constant motor activity is in turn dependent upon a constant inflow of sensory impulses into the cerebellum along its afferent connections, particularly upon the impulses from the vestibular portion of the internal ear, and those from the muscle sense fibers and similar fibers of so-called deep sensibility. The constant augmenting activity of the cerebellum is, therefore, a species of reflex effect, — a reflex tonus which affects all the musculature. Whether the cerebellar mechanism is especially arranged to co-ordinate its effect upon the neuro- muscular apparatus — that is, in some way to adapt the move- ments to a definite end — Luciani leaves an open question. He does not believe that a lack of co-ordination (cerebellar ataxia) is necessarily present in cerebellar lesions; but admits that, if this symptom is an invariable one, it would be necessary to add to the general augmenting activity of the cerebellum also a general adaptive or co-ordinating activity. It is precisely this latter feature which stands out in the minds of most physiologists as the characteristic function of the cerebellum, while Luciani considers that it is not demonstrated by clinical or experimental facts, and that even if demonstrated it would have to be considered as a part — perhaps a subordinate part — of the functional influence of this organ. Conclusions as to the General Functions of the Cerebel- lum.-— It is evident that an authoritative statement of the function or functions of the cerebellum is impossible. It seems quite clear, however, that the organ exerts a regulating influence of some kind upon the neuromuscular apparatus of our so-called voluntary movements. The precise nature of the regulating influence is in dispute, and one who reads the literature finds it difficult at times to separate clearly the different theories proposed, since some authors are content with general statements and others attempt a more specific analysis. On the whole, it seems desirable at present to hold to the general idea, introduced by Flourens, that the cerebellum is a central organ for co-ordination of voluntary movements, particularly the more complex movements necessaiy in equilibrium and locomotion. Instead, however, of assuming with Flourens that the cerebellum contains a co-ordinating principle, an expression that means nothing at present, we may assume that it exerts its co-ordinating influence by virtue of the definite nervous mechanisms contained in it — that is, by nervous complexes which, on the afferent side, are connected with the peripheral sensoiy 238 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. nerves to the vestibule of the ear, the muscles, joints, etc., and on the efferent side are in direct or indirect relations with the motor areas of the brain as well as the motor centers in the cord. These mechanisms are inherited structures, but, like other nervous mechan- isms, they are developed by use. The many muscular contractions made in our ordinary movements of equilibrium are learned by ex- perience, and the effects of this training are felt mainly upon these cerebellar paths or mechanisms. We regard the speech center in the cerebrum as a collection of nervous mechanisms in which are stored or preserved the connections necessary to the motor presentation of thoughts, a memory center for the spoken symbols of our con- cepts; it is possible that in the same way we may regard the cere- bellum as a memory center of the muscular movements concerned in equilibrium. The relations of the cerebellum with the motor areas of the cerebrum and the motor centers in the cord are evidently quite complex and far from being fully understood. Moreover, this relationship must vary considerably in different animals. Removal of the cerebrum from a pigeon leaves an animal with almost perfect power of controlling its equilibrium. In the dog a similar operation is followed by a longer period of inability to control perfectly the movements of locomotion, and it is probable that in man after such an operation the power of locomotion would be acquired more slowly, if at all. On the other hand, the violent effect upon such move- ii n nts caused by the removal of the cerebellum in the pigeon is less evident in the dog, and, if we may judge from the incomplete data of clinical neurology, very much less evident in man. In man the motor control of the voluntary muscular system through the cere- brum is more highly developed than in the lower animals. The most important objection to the view that the cerebellum is an organ of co-ordination for the movements of equilibrium and locomotion is that in the bird as well as in man the animal eventually learns to carry out these movements after loss of the cerebellum. This fad is clear proof that the cerebellum is not the only mechanism through which such co-ordination is possible; bu1 ii is no valid objection to the view that normally this control i- effected through this organ. The sensory tracts on which this co-ordination depends make connection with the thalamus or cere- brum as well as the cerebellum, and when the latter arc is broken the higher centen may be \\>«'(\ to replace its functions in part, at lea '. The replacement is not complete, since even in man loss of the cerebellum is followed by a permanent condition of slight ataxia. Lewandowsky's* suggestion that normally in man the finer, more con cious movements of the body are conl rolled direct ly * Lewandowsky, " Archiv f. Physiologie," L903, 1~">; see also Kohnstamm, "Archiv I. d. g< ammte Physiologie," 89, 240, 1902. CEREBELLUM, PONS, AND MEDULLA. 239 from the cerebrum, while the subconscious or dimly conscious movements of locomotion and equilibrium are regulated through the cerebellar centers seems to be in accord "with the facts known. The Psychical Functions of the Cerebellum. — In the cerebel- lum, as in the other nerve centers below the cerebrum, we have to consider the possibility of a psychical or conscious side to the activity of the organ. It seems clear, however, that the degree of conscious- ness, if any, exhibited by the cerebellum is of a much lower order than that shown by the cerebrum. All observers agree that there is no apparent loss of sensations after removal of the cerebellum, but Luciani, Russell, and others state their belief that in some indefinable way the mentality of the animal is affected by such operations. Whatever functions of this kind are present we can define only by the unsatisfactory term of subconscious rather than unconscious. As far as can be determined, this effect is felt mainly upon the muscular sense and the sense of direction. Localization of Function in the Cerebellum. — All observers agree that so far as the influence of the cerebellum on the muscula- ture of the body is concerned, it is homolateral, — that is, each half of the cerebellum is connected with its own half of the body. The connection with the motor areas of the brain is the reverse, the right half of the cerebrum being in relation with the left half of the cerebellum. These relations are, in the main, borne out by the anatomical course of the motor and sensory paths described above. There arises, however, the question whether or not there is a localization of function in the cere- bellum, that is, whether definite parts of the cerebellar cortex are in specific relations with separate muscles or groups of muscles. The possibility of a localization of function was suggested years ago by experiments made by Ferrier, in which electrical stimulation of the cortex gave definite movements of the head, limbs, and especially of the eyes, the movements varying somewhat according to the part stimulated. These results were not wholly confirmed by later observers. Horsley and Clarke* state that such strong stimuli are required to obtain a decisive effect from the cortex of the cerebellum that it may be questioned whether in positive cases the result is due to excita- tion of the cortex itself or to an escape of stimulus to the under- lying nuclei. Direct stimulation of the dentate nucleus gave them conjugate movements of the two eyes. These indications of a localization have been strengthened by the results of com- parative anatomy and especially by the effects of ablation of definite parts of the cortex. Earlier experimenters, using the * Horsley and Clarke, "Brain," 28, 13, 1905. 240 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. method of ablation, obtained quite negative results from the standpoint of localization, but this seems to have been due to the fact that a faulty anatomical schema was used; a whole hemi- sphere, or the entire vermiform lobe, etc., was removed. Later experimenters* have adopted the newer anatomical schemata, which take account of the true genetic relations of the various lobes and lobules of the cerebellum, and they have been rewarded by obtaining results of a positive character. The newer anatomical nomenclature is illustrated in Fig. 104, which gives a schematic representation of the arrangement of the lobules of the cere- bellum of the dog, according to Bolk. Following this schema van Rynberk reports that excision of the lobulus simplex is Lmjj Fig. 101. Schema of dog's cerebellum to show Bolk's nomenclature for the lobes and Blllci. Dorsal view : I. a, |c>l his anterior (this lobe is separated from the larger posterior lobe by the deep primary fissure, Svr) ; Ls, lobulus simplex; Lens, lobulus ansiformis; /./>, lobulus paramedianus ; /.»i/<, lobulus medianus posterior; AY, format is vermieularis (pars tonsillaris); C1, cms primum; '"-. crus secundum; Svr, sulcus primarius ; Sp, sulcus paramedianus ; Si, sulcus intercruralis. — (After van Rynberk.) followed by movements of the head (head nystagmus), which indicate an abnormal innervation of the neck muscles. Injury on on" side of t he crus primum of I he ansiform lobule is followed by abnormal movements of the forefoot of the same side, while similar injuries to the crus secundum result in abnormal move- ments localized to the hind fool. Extirpation of a lobulus paramedianus causes rolling movements round the long axis of the body or bending of the body to one side (pleurothotonus). It i- to be expected thai extension of this work will throw much lighl upon the Specific relations of the cortex of the cerebellum tot he musculat ure of I he body. The Medulla Oblongata. In the medulla oblongata we must recognize a region of special physiological importance in that it • Van Rynberk, "General Review in Ergebnisse der Physiologic, " 7, 663, L908. 5 6 FijE. 105 Nuclei of origin of motor and primary terminal sen ory nuclei of cerebral nerve {Held): Bchematicallj lepresented in a supposedly transparent brain stem viewed from behind. (Nuclei and rool <>i motor nerves in light red, <>f sensory nerves in purple. Cochlear nerve in yellow.) 4, nucleus of the third nerve (n. oculomotorii); ."), nucleus of the fourth nerva (n. troehlearis); 6, the fourth nerve; 7, the descending (motor) nmt of the fifth nerve; 8, the principal motor nucleus of the fifth nerve; 'i, the semilunar ganglion (g. Gasseri); 26, the ascending (sensory) n>"t of the fifth nerve; 14, nucleus "C the ixth cranial nerve; l">, nucleus of the facial (seventh) nerve; In, the fm-i.-i I nerve; 34, 33, nucleus "f the vestibular branch of the eighth omnia) nerve; 32, ventral nucleus of the cochlear branch of the eighth nerve; 27, dorsal nucleus of the cocnleai branch of the eighth nerve; 19, -'K the glossopharyngeal nerve; 18, 28, the nerve; 20, motor nuclei r,f vagus and glossopharyngeal (nucleus ambiguus and nucleus dorsalis); Si. 24, nucleu of the ake cinereas, the solitary bundle and its nuclei] ; 17, the eleventh or spinal accessory nerve; 22. nucleus of the spinal accessory; 21, nucleus of the hypoglot al nerve, (From SpaUeholz, Human Anatomy.") CEREBELLUM, PONS, AND MEDULLA. 241 is the seat of certain centers which control the activity of the circulatory and respiratory organs. If the medulla is severed from the portion of the brain tying anterior to it the animal con- tinues to live for a considerable period. The respiratory move- ments are performed rhythmically, and the blood-vessels retain their tone so as to maintain an approximately normal blood-pressure. On the contrary, destruction of the medulla, or severance of its connections with the underlying parts, is followed by a cessation of respiration and a loss of tone in the arteries, either of which results in the rapid death of the organism as a whole. The portions of the medulla which exercise these important functions are desig- nated, respectively, as the respiratory and the vasomotor or vaso- constrictor centers. Their location and to some extent their con- nections have been determined by physiological experiments, but so far it has not been possible to mark out histologically the exact groups of cells concerned. The position and physiological properties of these centers are described in the sections on respiration and circulation. These centers are of especial importance because of their wide connections with the body, their essentially independent activity in reference to the higher parts of the brain, and the abso- lutely necessary character of the regulations they effect. In the development of the brain the functions originally mediated by the lower parts have been transferred more and more to the higher parts, especially in regard to conscious sensation and motion, and the so-called higher psychical activities. But the unconscious and involuntary regulation of the organs of circulation and respiration and to a certain extent of the other visceral organs has been cen- tralized, as it were, in the medulla. In addition to the control of the respiration and circulation other important reflex activities are effected through the medulla by means of the vagus nerve, which has its nucleus of origin in this part of the brain. Such, for instance, are the reflex control of the heart through the cardio- inhibitory center and of the motions and secretions of the alimentary canal. The Nuclei of Origin and the Functions of the Cranial Nerves. — The origin, course, anatomical and physiological relations of the first or olfactory, second or optic, and eighth or auditor}'' nerves have been referred to in the preceding pages. For the sake of completeness the origin and functions of the other cranial nerves may be summarized briefly in this connection. The Third Cranial Nerve (N. Ocidomotorius) . — This nerve arises from the base of the brain on the median side of the corresponding pedunculus cerebri. It is a motor nerve supplying fibers to four of the extrinsic muscles of the eyeballs — namely, the internal rectus, the superior rectus, the inferior rectus, and the inferior 16 242 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. oblique — and to the levator palpebral It innervates also two important intrinsic muscles of the eyeball, the ciliary muscle used in accommodating the eye in near vision, and the sphincter of the iris, which controls in part the size of the pupil. These two latter muscles belong to the type of plain muscle, and the fibers of the third nerve which innervate them terminate in the ciliary ganglion, whence the path is continued by sym- pathetic nerve fibers (postganglionic fibers) to the muscles. In the interior of the brain the fibers of the third nerve arise from a conspicuous nucleus or collection of nuclei situated in the cen- Edinger-Westphal nucleus. Principal nucleus Median nucleus. Nucleus of 4th nerve. Fig. 106. Nuclei of origin of the third and fourth nerves. — (From 1'oirit r nnd Cluirpii.) tral gray matter of the midbrain at the level of the superior col- liculus. The fibers for the ciliary muscle and sphincter pupillse arise more anteriorly than those for the extrinsic muscles. His- tologically three parts at least may be distinguished, as shown in Fig. 106, namely, the lateral (or principal) nucleus, which gives origin chiefly to the fibers innervating the extrinsic muscles; the median nucleus: and t he nucleus of Edinger-Westphal. According to Bernheimer* the large median nucleus gives rise to the fibers that innervate the ciliary muscles, while the Edinger-Westphal nuclei (accessory nuclei) emit nil i he movements of the sphincter muscle of the iris. Some of the fibers, particularly those from * Bernheimer, in " Graefe Saemi cb't I iandbuch derges. Augenheilkunde," 2d ed., I, 41. CEREBELLUM, PONS, AND MEDULLA. 243 the lateral nucleus to the inferior rectus, the internal rectus, and the inferior oblique, cross the mid-line and emerge in the nerve of the opposite side. Fig. 107. — Diagram showing the average area of distribution of the sensory fibers of the trigeminal nerve. — (Cushing.) N. opht, N. max. sup. N. max. inf. Fig. 108. — Nuclei of origin of the fifth cranial nerve. — (From Poirier and Charpy, after Van Gehuchten.) The Fourth Cranial Nerve (N. Trochlearis) . — This nerve emerges from the brain in the anterior medullary velum (valve of Vieussens) just posterior to the inferior colliculus. It curves around the 244 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. pedunculus cerebri to reach the base of the brain. It is a motor nerve, and supplies fibers to the superior oblique muscle of the eyeball. In the interior of the brain the fibers arise from a nucleus in the central gray matter just posterior to that of the third nerve (Fig. 106). The fibers pass dorsalward toward the vein in and make a complete decussation before emerging. The Fifth Cranial Nerve (N. Trigeminus) . — This nerve arises from the side of the pons by two roots, a small motor root, portio minor, and a large sensory root, portio major. It is, therefore, a mixed motor and sensory nerve, supplying motor fibers to the muscles of mastication and sensory fibers of pressure, pain, and temperature to the face, the forepart of the scalp, the eye, nose, portions of the ear, mouth, and tongue, and to the dura mater (Fig. 107). In the interior of the brain the motor portion, portio minor, arises partly from a small nucleus in the pons and partly from a long column of cells extending along the lower margin of the central gray matter throughout the midbrain. This column and the fibers arising from it constitute the descending motor root of the fifth nerve (see Fig. 108). The sensory fibers originate from the nerve cells in the Gasserian ganglion (g. semilunare). The branch that enters the brain ends partly in a collection of cells in the pons, the so-called sensory nucleus, and partly in a column of cells extending posteriorly throughout the length of the medulla. These cells and the fibers ending in them constitute the descending spinal root of the fifth nerve (see Fig. 105). The Sixth Cranial Nerve (N. Abducens). — This nerve arises from the base of the brain at the posterior edge of the pons. It is a motor nerve, and supplies fibers to the external rectus muscle of the eye- ball. In the interior of the brain its fibers originate in a small spheri- cal nucleus lying beneath the floor of the fourth ventricle. Con- nections have been traced between this nucleus and the pyramidal tract of the opposite side (Fig. 105). The Seventh Cranial Nerve (N. Facialis). — This nerve appears on t he base of the brain at i he inferior margin of the p. '.». tScli.it'' I' I -hunk of Physiology," 1900, vol. ii ; "10rgcbnis.se der iologie," 1903, vol. ii, part n, j). 823 ; also "Brain," 1903, vol. xxvi. 240 SYMPATHETIC NERVOUS SYSTEM. 247 c& Cord ^UC Qjymjbatkeiic lion- Tbst-oanfflion.i(t fibre. Plain, I muscle. Autonomic Nervous System. — According to Langley, the efferent fibers from the nerve cells of the sympathetic and re- lated ganglia supply the plain muscle tissues, the cardiac muscles, and the glands, — that is, the organs of the involuntary or, according to an old nomenclature, the vegetative processes of the body. He proposes for this entire sys- tem of efferent fibers the term autonomic, to indicate that the}' possess a certain independ- ence of the central nervous system. The au- tonomic system is contrasted physiologically and anatomically with the efferent spinal and cranial fibers that supply the striated or volun- tary muscles: physiologically in the fact that this latter group of fibers is entirely dependent upon activities of the central nervous system, and anatomically in the fact that the auto- nomic fibers, although arising ultimately from the central nervous system, all pass to their pe- ripheral tissues by way of sympathetic nerve cells. The autonomic path consists of two neurons : one belonging to the central nervous system, whose axon emerges in one of the spinal or cranial nerves and ends around the dendrites of a sympathetic cell; and one occurring in some one of the numerous sympathetic ganglia, whose axon passes to the peripheral tissue. The first axon is spoken of as the preganglionic fiber, the second as the postganglionic fiber. Their con- nections are represented in the accompanying schema (Fig. 109). Physiological and anatomical investigations have shown that autonomic nerve fibers arise from four regions in the central nervous system (Fig. 1 10) : First, from the midbrain, emerging Preganglionic jibre. Fig. 109. — Schema to show the general relation between the preganglionic and postganglionic fibers of the autonomic paths. Fig. 110.— Illus- trating the central ori- gin of the autonomic fibers. — {Langley.) 248 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. in the third cranial nerve and passing via the ciliary ganglion; second, from the bulbar region, emerging in the seventh, ninth, and tenth cranial nerves; third, from the thoracic spinal nerves (first thoracic to fourth or fifth lumbar) and passing in general via the ganglia of the sympathetic chain; fourth, from the sacral ' region by way of the so-called nervus erigens supplying the descending colon, rectum, anus, and genital organs. The autonomic fibers at their origin in the central nervous system — that is, while pregan- glionic fibers — are all possessed of a small medullated sheath, having a diameter of 1.8 [J. to 4 fx. The postganglionic fiber is in most cases non-medullated, but this is by no means an invariable rule. In many cases the axons from sympathetic cells possess distinct, although small, myelin sheaths. The Nicotin Method. — The course of the autonomic fibers has been traced in many cases to their corresponding sympathetic nerve cells partly by the method of secondary degeneration and partly by the use of nicotin, as first described by Langley and Dickinson.* These authors have shown that after the use of nicotin, either injected into the circulation or painted upon the ganglion, stimulation of the preganglionic fiber in any part of its course fails to give any response, while stimulation of the post- ganglionic fiber, on the contrary, is still effective. It would seem, therefore, that the nicotin paralyzes the connection of the pre- ganglionic fiber with the sympathetic nerve cell, and by means of the local application of the drug it is possible in many cases to pick out the ganglion in which the preganglionic fiber really ends. For it often happens that in the sympathetic trunk a preganglionic fiber will pass through several ganglia before making final connections with a sympathetic cell. So far, I lie course of these fibers has been traced most, successfully in the case of the nerves supplying the sweat-glands, blood-vessels, and especially the erector muscles of the hairs, the so-called pilomotor nerve-fibers. The visible result of stimulation in the last case gives a ready means of determining the presence of the fibers. General Course of the Autonomic Fibers Arising from the Spinal Nerves. — It has long been known that the spinal nerves .in- connected with many of the ganglia of the sympathetic chain by fine branches known as the rami rommunicantes. In the tho- racic and lumbar regions (first thoracic to second or fourth lumbar) these rami consist of two parts, a white and a gray ramus, the difference in color being due to (lie fact, that I he white rami are composed almost entirely of medullated libers, while the gray rami are largely non-medullated. In the cervical, lower lumbar, and sacral regions the rami consist only of tin; gray part. Physiological ♦ "Proceedings, Royal Society," 1889, 46, 428. SYMPATHETIC NERVOUS SYSTEM. 249 experiments show that the white rami consist of preganglionic fibers that arise from nerve cells in the spinal cord, pass out by way of the anterior roots, enter the white ramus, and thus reach the sympathetic chain. On entering this latter the fiber may not end at once in the ganglion at which it enters, but may pass up or down in the chain for some distance. Eventually, however, it ends around a sympathetic nerve cell and the path is then con- tinued by the axon from this cell as the postganglionic fiber. The gray rami consist of these latter fibers, which return from the sym- pathetic chain to the spinal nerves and are then distributed to the areas supplied by these nerves, particularly the cutaneous areas, since the skin branches are the ones that supply the sweat glands, the blood-vessels, and the erector muscles of the hairs. It will be noted that the fibers that pass from a given spinal nerve — say, the twelfth thoracic — by a white ramus to enter the sympathetic chain do not return as postganglionic fibers by the gray ramus to the same spinal nerve. On the contrary, the gray ramus of the twelfth thoracic may consist of the postganglionic portion of autonomic fibers that enter the sympathetic through a white ramus of one of the higher thoracic nerves. In general, we may say that there is a great outflow of autonomic fibers, including vasomotor, sweat, and pilomotor fibers, in the white rami commu- nicantes from the first or second thoracic to the second or fourth lumbar nerves. Those of these fibers that are to be distributed to the skin areas of the body— head, limbs, and trunk — return by way of the gray rami to the various spinal nerves and are distributed with these nerves, the distribution being somewhat different in different animals and for the different varieties of fibers. Those fibers that are distributed eventually to the blood-vessels, glands, and walls of the viscera have a different course from those supplying the glands, blood-vessels, and plain muscle of the head region. For the head region the fibers after entering the sympathetic chain pass upward along the cervical sympathetic to end in the superior cervical ganglion; thence the path is continued by postganglionic fibers which emerge by the. various plexuses that arise from this ganglion. For the abdominal and pelvic viscera the fibers (particu- larly the rich supply of vasoconstrictor fibers), after entering the sympathetic chain, emerge, still as preganglionic fibers, by the splanchnic nerves that run to the celiac ganglion or in the branches connecting with the inferior mesenteric ganglia, and then become postganglionic fibers (see Fig. 111). The details of the course of the vasomotor, sweat, visceromotor fibers to the different regions, the cardiac fibers, etc., will be given in the appropriate sections. General Course of the Autonomic Fibers Arising from the Brain. — These fibers leave the brain in the third, seventh, ninth, 250 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL XERYOUS SYSTEM. tenth, and eleventh cranial nerves. Fig. 111. — Diagram (riving a schematic representation of tin- coin e of the autonomic 1«ym jKit hi-i \r) fibers arising from the I boracico- ombar and acral regions of the cool, i be preganglionic fiber la represented in red, the postganglionic in black lines. The arrow in* dieate the normal direction of tin; nerve im- i>ui es or nerve conduction, 8.e., Superior cervical ganglion; l.r., inferior cervical gan- glion; T, the fii I thoracic ganglion ; 8p., the splanchnic nerve; ''., tin; .semilunar or celiac ganglion ; m., t be inferior mesenteric ganglion ; A., the hypogastric nerves; N.K., the nervu eriRens. the numerals indicate the corre- sponding spinal net Those that emerge in the third nerve end, as preganglionic fi- bers, in the ciliary ganglion. Their postganglionic fibers leave this ganglion in the short ciliary nerves and innervate the plain muscle of the sphinc- ter of the iris and the ciliary muscle. The fibers that emerge in the seventh and ninth nerves probably supply the glands and blood-vessels (vaso- dilator fibers) of the mucous membrane of the nose and mouth. Some of these fibers reach the fifth nerve by way of anastomosing branches and are distributed \vith it. Their preganglionic portion termi- nates in some of the ganglia belonging to the sympathetic typo which arc found in this region, such as the sphenopal- atine and otic ganglia, and the submaxillary and sublingual ganglia for the fibers dis- tributed to the glands of the same name. The autonomic fibers that arise with the tenth (and the eleventh) nerves are distributed through the vagus. Physiologically these fibers consist of motor fibers (vis- ceromotor fibers) to the mus- culature of the esophagus, stomach, small intestine, and large intestine as far as the descending colon, motor fibers to the bronchial musculature, inhibitory fibers to the heart, and secretory fibers to the gastric and pancreatic glands. The ganglia in which the pre- ganglionic portions end have not been definitely located, SYMPATHETIC NERVOUS SYSTEM. 251 but probably they comprise the small and, for the most part, un- named local ganglia found in or near the organs innervated. General Course of the Autonomic Fibers Arising from the Sacral Cord. — The autonomic fibers of this region emerge from the cord in the anterior roots of the sacral nerves, — second to fourth. The branches from these roots unite to form the so-called nervus erigens (pelvic nerve) , which loses itself in the pelvic plexus without making connections with the sympathetic chain of ganglia. The pelvic plexus is formed in part also from the hypogastric nerve arising from the inferior mesenteric ganglion. Through this latter path autonomic fibers from the upper lumbar region enter the plexus (Fig. 111). The autonomic fibers of the nervus erigens suppl}T vasodilator fibers to the external genital organs, and in the male constitute the physiological mechanism for erection; whence the name. They supply, also, vasodilator fibers to rectum and anus and motor fibers to the plain muscles of the colon de- scendens, rectum, and anus. The preganglionic parts of these fibers end in small sympathetic ganglia in the pelvic plexus or in the neighborhood of the organs supplied. Normal Mode of Stimulation of the Autonomic Nerve Fibers. • — In distinction from the nerve fibers innervating the skeletal muscles practically the whole set of autonomic fibers is removed 'from the control of the will. An apparent exception to this general statement is found in the fact that the ciliary muscle of the eye is seemingly under voluntary control. We must suppose that under normal conditions the autonomic fibers are always excited reflexly, and the course of the afferent fibers concerned in these reflexes and the nature of the effective sensory stimulus in each case are important in the consideration of each of the physiological mechanisms involved. Most of these mechanisms, as we shall find, work reflexly — that is, without voluntary initiation — and, for the most part, unconsciously, for instance, the movements of the intestines, the secretion of the digestive glands, and the contraction and dilatation of the arteries. The autonomic nerve-fibers control, therefore, the uncon- scious co-ordinated actions, the so-called vegetative processes, of the body. There is no apparent reason in the anatomical ar- rangements why these fibers should be free from voluntary control. Their distinguishing characteristic in comparison with the nerves for the voluntary movements is the fact that they all terminate first in sympathetic nerve cells; but this fact gives no explanation of the absence of conscious control by the will. We are justified in saying that nerve paths that pass through sympathetic nerve cells cannot be excited voluntarily; but the immediate reason for this fact is probably to be found in the ultimate point of origin of these 252 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. paths in the central nervous system. What we designate as vol- untary motor paths arise in a definite region of the cortex, — the motor area in the frontal lobe. Our motor conceptions or ideas can affect the efferent paths arising in this region, but not those, apparently, which originate in other parts of the brain. CHAPTER XIII. THE PHYSIOLOGY OF SLEEP. The state of more or less complete unconsciousness which we designate as sleep forms a part of the physiology of the brain which naturally has attracted much attention, and the theoretical explana- tions that have been advanced at one time or another are exceed- ingly numerous. The same condition occurs in many, if not all, of the other mammalia, and, indeed, in all living things there occur periods of rest alternating with periods of activity. Whether these periods of rest are essentially similar in nature to sleep in man is a question in general physiology that can be solved only when we know more of the chemistry of living matter. Within the human body there are other tissues that exhibit periods of rest alternating with periods of activity, — the gland cells, for example. The secret- ing cells of the pancreas have a period of activity in which the destructive processes exceed the constructive, and a period of rest in which these relations are reversed. We may compare this con- dition in the gland cells with that in the brain. Sleep, from this standpoint, is a period of comparative rest or inactivity, during which the constructive or anabolic processes are in excess of the disassimilatory changes. The period of sleep is a period of re- cuperation, and doubtless all tissues have these alternating phases. To explain sleep fundamentally, therefore, it would be necessary to understand the chemical changes of anabolism and catabolism, and an explanation of the sleep of the brain tissues would doubtless explain the similar phenomenon in other tissues. But what the physiologists desire first, and have attempted to determine, is an explanation of why this condition comes on with a certain periodical regularity, — an explanation, in other words, of the mechanism of sleep, the change or changes in the brain or the body which reduce the metabolism of the brain tissue to such an extent that it falls below the level necessary to cause conscious- ness. Physiological Relations during Sleep. — The central and most important fact of sleep is the partial or complete loss of conscious- ness, and this phenomenon may be referred directly to a lessened metabolic activity in the brain tissue, presumably in the cortex cerebri. During sleep the following changes have been recorded: 253 254 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. The respirations become slower and deeper and the costal respiration (respiration by elevation of the ribs) predominates over the ab- dominal or diaphragmatic respiration as compared with the waking condition. The respiratory movements also show frequently a tendency to become periodic, — that is, to increase and decrease regularly in groups after the manner of the Cheyne-Stokes type of breathing. The expiration is frequently shorter and more audi- ble than in the respirations of the waking hours. The eyeballs roll upward and inward and the pupil is constricted. According to Lombard's observations, the knee-kick decreases or disappears entirely during sleep. Some of the constant secretions are dimin- ished in amount, — as, for instance, the urine, the tears, and the secretion of the mucous glands in the nasal or pharyngeal mem- brane. One of the familiar signs of a sleepy condition is the dryness of the surface of the eyes, a condition that leads to the rubbing of the eyes. It is sometimes stated that the digestive secretions are diminished during sleep, but the statement does not seem to rest upon satisfactory observations, and may be doubted. The pulse-rate decreases during sleep and there are also certain sig- nificant changes in the distribution of blood in the body owing to a diminished vascular tone in the skin vessels. These latter changes will be referred to more in detail below. The physiological oxidations are also decreased, as shown by the diminished output of carbon dioxid. On the whole, however, the physiological activities of the body go on much as in the waking condition. Those changes in activity that do occur are, in the main, an indirect result of the partial or complete cessation of activity in the brain. One might say that while the cortex of the brain sleeps — that is, is inactive — most of the other organs of the body may be awake and maintain their normal activity. Another fact of interest is that the entile cortex docs not fall asleep at the same instant nor always to the same extent. Ordinarily as sleep sets in the power to make conscious movements is lost first and the auditory sen- sibility last, and on awakening the reverse relation holds. The individual may be conscious of sound sensations before he is sufficiently awake to make voluntary movements. The Intensity of Sleep. The intensity of sleep that is, the depth of unconsciousness -has been studied by the simple device jcertaining the intensity of th< sensory stimulus necessary to awaken the sleeper. Kohlschlitter * used for this purpose a pendu- lum falling againsl a sounding plate. At intervals of a half-hour during the period of sleep the auditory stimuli thus produced were increased inintensitj until waking was caused. His results arc expressed in the curve shown in Fig. I 11^, in which the intensity * Kohlschutter, "ZeitBchrift f, rationelle Medicin," 1863. THE PHYSIOLOGY OF SLEEP. 255 of the sleep is represented by the height of the ordinates. According to this curve, the greatest intensity is reached about an hour after the beginning, and from the second to the third hour onward the depth of sleep is very slight. The activities of the brain lie just oelow the threshold of consciousness. It appears also from this curve that the recuperative effect of sleep is not proportional to its intensity. The long period from the third to the eighth hour, in which the depth of sleep is so slight is presumably as important in restoring the brain to its normal waking irritability as the deeper STRENGTH OF STIMULUS 800 700 / \ i / GGO 500 400 ' - ■ 300 " \ ZOO 100 \ \ ' 1 ___ HOURS 05 LO 1.5 Z.O 2.5 3.0 3.5 4.0 45 5.0 5.5 6.0 S£ 7.0 7.5 7.8 Fig. 112. — Curve illustrating the strength of an auditory stimulus (a ball falling from a height) necessary to awaken a sleeping person. The hours marked below. The tests were made at half-hour intervals. The curve indicates that the distance through which it was necessary to drop the ball increased during the first hour, and then diminished, at first very rapidly, then slowly. — (Kohlschutter.) period up to the third hour. That this is the case is perhaps sufficiently demonstrated by the experience of every one, but Weygandt has attempted to prove the point by direct experi- ments. He found that for simple mental acts, such as the ad- dition of pairs of figures, a short sleep was as effective as a longer one, but for more difficult mental work, such as memorizing groups of ten figures, efficiency was distinctly improved in proportion to the length of sleep. It is probable that the curve of intensity of sleep varies somewhat with the individual and also with surrounding conditions. That individual variations occur is indicated by the results obtained by two other observers, Monninghoff and Piesbergen,* who used the same general method as was employed by Koklschiitter. The sleeper was awakened by auditory stimuli produced by dropping a lead * Monninghoff and Piesbergen, " Zeitschrift f. Biologie," 19, 1, 1883. 256 PHYSIOLOGY OF CENTRAL NERVOUS SYSTEM. ball from varying heights upon a lead plate. Only two experi- ments were made each night, and the curves constructed repre- sent, therefore, composites from several periods of sleep. One of the curves obtained is represented in Fig. 113. According to this curve the maximum intensity is reached between the first and second hours, and between the fourth and the fifth hour there is a second slight increase in intensity, giving a second maximum in the curve. This latter feature of a second increase in intensity toward morning is very apparent also in some interesting curves obtained by Czerny from children of different ages. His method of awakening the sleeper was to use induction shocks of varying intensities. In children of four years with a normal period of sleep of about twelve hours the curve shows a very marked increase in intensity toward morning, as shown in Fig. 114. Curves made by similar experimental methods are reported by Howell and by Michelson.* The striking feature about all the curves is the sharp increase in intensity shortly after falling asleep; in most cases the maximum is reached at the first or second hour of slumber, but Michelson believes that there are two classes of individuals in this respect, those with morning dispositions in whom the maximum of mental efficiency occurs early in the day and who upon going to sleep show a maximum of intensity within an hour, and those with evening dispositions whose maximum efficiency comes later in the day and whose curve of sleep reaches its maximum of intensity with relative slowness (If to 3^ hrs.). Changes in the Circulation during Sleep. — That the circula- tion undergoes distinct and characteristic changes during sleep has been shown upon man by phlethysmographic observations and upon the lower animals by direct kymographic experiments. Using very young dogs, Tarchanofrj" has been able to measure their blood-pressure while sleeping. He finds that (he pressure in the aorta falls by an amount equal to twenty to fifty millimeters of mercury during sleep, and that the same general fact is true for man is shown by the sphygmomanometric observations reported by Brush and layrrucal her.} Making use of patients with a trephine hole in the skull, Mosso# has been able to show that during Bleep the volume of the brain diminishes, while that of the arm or foot increases. The apparent explanation of this fact is that the blood-vessels in the body dilate, and receive, therefore, more * Howell, "Journal of Experimental Medicine," 2, 313, 1897. Michel- son, "Dissertation," Dorpat, L891. ; Tarchanoff, "Archives italiennes de biologie," 21, 318, L894. I Bru li ;inr mediates its own specific, quality of sensation, the specific energy of the optic apparatus being visual sensations, of (lie auditor)- apparatus sound sensations, etc., and each sensory nerve or apparatus can give no oilier than its own quality of sensation. Whether this specificity in die reaction of each sensory nerve is due to some pecu- liarity in the nerve it-elf <.r iis peripheral end-organ, or to a, pecu liarity of the part of the brain in which it terminates Midler left, an open question, although he called attention to the fact that the central ending is capable of giving its specific effect, in con- CLASSIFICATION OF THE SENSES. 267 sciousness independently of the conducting nerve fibers. With regard to this latter question the opinions of physiologists still differ. Most physiologists, perhaps, adopt the view that the specific reaction in consciousness is due to the central ending, — that, in other words, the different sensory parts of the cortex give different kinds or qualities of consciousness, while the sensory nerve fibers are simply conductors of nerve impulses, which, however much they may differ in intensity, are qualitatively the same in all nerve fibers. According to this view, it would result, as du Bois-Reymond expressed it, that, if the auditory nerve fibers were attached to the visual center and the optic fibers to the auditory center, we would see the thunder and hear the lightning. Each typical sense-organ from this standpoint consists of three essential parts: the central ending, which deter- mines the quality of the sensation; the peripheral end-organ, retina, cochlea, etc., which determines whether or not any given form of stimulus shall be effective and which in most cases is con- structed so as to be responsive to a special form of stimulus desig- nated as its adequate stimulus; and of connecting neurons whose only function is to conduct the nerve impulses originating in the end-organ. The fact, therefore, that the light waves can stimulate the rods and cones of the retina, but are an inadequate stimulus probably to the hair cells of the cochlea or the taste buds of the tongue, is due to a peculiarity in structure of the rods and cones; but the fact that the impulses conducted by the optic fibers arouse a peculiar modality of sensation is not due to any peculiarity in structure in these fibers or in the rods and cones, but to a charac- teristic structure of the optic centers. The positive experimental evidence for the correctness of this view is not conclusive, but, on the whole, is impressive. Such facts as the following may be noted: 1 . When sensory nerve fibers are stimulated otherwise than through their end-organs each reacts, if it reacts at all, according to its specific energy, — that is, it produces its own quality of sensa- tion. When the optic nerve is cut, for instance, the mechanical stimulus causes a flash of light ; when the chorda tympani is stimu- lated in the tympanic cavity by mechanical, electrical, or chemical stimuli sensations of taste are aroused. 2. Mechanical pressure upon the peripheral nerves distributed to the skin may cause a loss of some of the cutaneous senses in certain areas of the skin with a retention of others. Thus the senses of pressure and temperature may be lost and that of pain retained, or pain may be lost and pressure retained. A similar dissocia- tion of the sensations of the skin in definite regions may be observed after localized lesions of the spinal cord, or during the process of regeneration that follows suture of a severed nerve. Such facts agree with the view that each sense has its own set 268 THE SPECIAL SENSES. of nerve-fibers; those that mediate pain cannot by a mere modification of the stimulus give also a sense of pressure. 3. The only objective manifestation of a nerve impulse that we can study in the nerve itself is the electrical change that accom- panies it or that perhaps constitutes its essence. This electrical change is qualitatively the same in all kinds of nerve fibers, and this fact agrees with the view that the nerve impulse is qualitatively the same in all fibers. So far as the sensory nerve fibers are concerned, the chief ob- jection to this view of the doctrine of specific nerve energies is found perhaps in the difficulty or impossibility of applying it to the explanation of color vision. According to the strict interpreta- tion of the view, each fundamental color sense, being distinct in quality, should be mediated by its own set of nerve fibers. When Hehnholtz first formulated his theory of color vision he spoke, therefore, of three kinds of nerve fibers, — the red, the green, and the violet, — each when stimulated alone giving its own specific sensation and not capable of giving any other. The facts accumu- lated regarding color vision, however, seem to show that this view will not hold. One and the same cone, with its connecting fiber, may give rise to any or all of the primary color sensations, and, unless we choose to further subdivide the nerve unit and assume that the separate nerve fibrils of which the axis cylinder is composed constitute the separate conductors for the primary sense qualities, it would seem to be impossible to apply the doctrine of specific energies to this case. Not too much weight should be given per- haps to this objection. For it must be remembered that all of our present theories of color vision are unsatisfactory, and possibly when we attain to the light, point of view the facts may not be so difficult to interpret in terms of this theory of specific energies. The alternative view proposed in place of the doctrine of specific nerve energies assumes that the nerve impulses may vrarj in quality .-is well as in intensity, and that therefore one and the 3ame nerve fiber may arouse different qualities of sensation and different end effects according to the character of Hie impulse conveyed. This point, of view is not capable of much discussion, since there are no positive facts that support- it. It is logically actory in meeting the cases in which the former view seems to be unsatisfactory. It, is difficult, however, in our ignorance of the nature of the nerve impulse to imagine in what respects it may : >lv differ in character. The Weber-Fechner (Psychophysical) Law.— One difficulty thai has been encountered in Hie physiological study of sensory nerve i thai the end reaction cannot be measured with exactness. With efferenl nervee the end reaction is a contraction or secretion CLASSIFICATION OF THE SENSES. 269 that can be estimated quantitatively in terms of our physical and chemical units of measurement. But the end reaction of a sensory nerve is a state of consciousness for which we have no standard of measurement. Weber, in studying the relation between the strength of the stimulus and the amount of the resulting sensation, availed himself of the method of the least detectible change in sen- sation; that is, he determined the increase in stimulus at different levels necessary to cause a just perceptible increase in the sensation. By means of this method he arrived at the significant result that the increase in stimulus necessary to cause this change is, within physiological limits, a definite fractional increment of the acting stimulus. If, for instance, with a weight of 30 gms. upon the finger it requires an increment of -^ — that is, one additional gram — to make a just perceptible difference in the pressure sensa- ,-- -" ~/7. 75 II „.- '-' ' /3. 3 9 3 7 --' **" 70. 90 ^' I 5-0 / S .63 / / '* 11 T / / 3.1 6 3 1 / z. 37 I > ■77 i 1.3 3 i / 1 1 0 6 / Z I K * f- J i ■> / 7 b 5 A 1 / 1 / z / ? /' / / 5" It ' /; ' it r EXCITATION^. Fig. 117. — Curve to indicate the Weber-Fechner law of a logarithmical relation be- tween excitation and sensation. — (From Waller.) The excitations are indicated along the abscissas, the sensations along the ordinates. The increase in sensation is represented as tak- ing place in equal steps, "the minimal perceptible difference," while the corresponding excitations require an increasing increment of i at each step, namely 1, 1.33, 1.77, 2.37, etc. That is, for equal increments of sensation increasing increments of stimulation are necessary. tion, then, with a weight of 60 gms. upon the finger the addition of another gram would not be perceived ; it would require again an increment of -^ — that is, 2 gms. — to make a just perceptible dif- ference in sensation. This relationship is known as Weber's law. While its exactness has often been disputed, it seems to be generally admitted that for a median range of stimulation the law expresses the approximate relation between the two variables considered. Fechner attempted to give this law a more quantitative and ex- tensive application by assuming that just perceptible differences 270 THE SPECIAL SENSES. in sensation represent actually equal amounts of sensation. Ac- cepting this assumption, we can express the relationship between stimulus and sensation as determined by Weber's experiments by saying that for the sensation to increase by equal amounts, — that is, by arithmetical progression, — the stimulus must vary according to a certain factor,— that is, by geometrical progression. The sensation may be regarded as a geometrical function of the stimulus. If the relation between stimulus and sensation is repre- sented as a curve in which the ordinates express the sensation in- creasing by equal amounts, and the abscissas the corresponding stimuli increasing at each interval by -3-, a result is obtained such as is represented in the accompanying figure (Fig. 117). A curve of this kind is a logarithmical curve, and Fechner expressed the rela- tionship between stimulus and sensation in what has been called the psychophysical law, — namely, that the sensation varies as the logarithm of the stimulus. From the physiological standpoint it is important to bear in mind, as has been emphasized by Waller,* that several steps intervene between the action of the external stimulus and the production of the conscious sensation. The external stim- ulus acts first on the end-organ, this in turn upon the sensory nerve fiber, producing a nerve impulse which finally in the brain gives the conscious reaction. It is a question, therefore, whether the logarith- mical relation of the stimulus holds between it and the reaction of the end-organ or between the internal stimulus — that is, the sensory nerve impulse — and the psychical reaction. This author has given some facts obtained by recording the action current in the optic nerve, the retina being stimulated by known intensities of light, which indicate that the relation observed is between the external stimulus and the internal stimulus, — that is, the sensory nerve impulse. ♦Waller, "Bruin," 201, 1895. CHAPTER XV. CUTANEOUS AND INTERNAL SENSATIONS. General Classification. — According to the older views, the sensory nerves of the skin give sensations of touch. Modern physiology has shown, however, that these nerves mediate at least four different qualities of sensation — namely, pressure, warmth, cold, and pain. Our so-called touch sensations are usually compound, consisting of a pressure and a temperature component and also very frequently an element of muscle sense when muscular efforts are involved, as, for instance, in measuring weights or resistances. The four sensory qualities enumerated constitute the cutaneous senses, and they are present, or, to speak more accurately, the nerves through which these senses are mediated are present not only over the general cutaneous surface but also in those membranes — such as the mucous membrane of the mouth and the rectum (stomodeum and proctodeum) — which embryologically are formed from the epiblast. The surfaces in the interior of the body, on the contrary — such as the membranes of the alimentary canal, muscles, fascise, etc. — have only nerves of pain, but no sense of touch or temperature. Of these cutaneous senses, three — pressure, warmth, and cold — may be grouped with the exterior senses, the sensations being projected to the exterior of the body, into the substance causing the stimulation; although, as was mentioned above, the temperature sensations under con- ditions— fever, vascular dilatation, etc. — may be projected to parts of the skin itself and be felt as changes in ourselves. The temperature sensations are, in fact, projected to the exterior whenever they are combined with pressure sensations, the latter serving, as it were, as the dominant sense. The pain sense, on the other hand, belongs to the group of interior senses, the sensations being always projected into our own body and being felt as changes in ourselves. Protopathic, Epicritic, and Deep Sensibility. — In the matter of the classification of the cutaneous senses and, indeed, the body senses in general, a new point of view has been suggested by Head and Rivers.* These authors made a careful study of the loss of sensations after division of the cutaneous nerves, * Head and Rivers, "Brain," 1905, 99, and 1908, 323. 271 272 THE SPECIAL SENSES. and of the subsequent gradual and separate return of these sensations following upon suture of the divided ends. They find that in skin areas made completely anesthetic there is present a deep or subcutaneous sensibility to pressure and movements, a sensibility which must be mediated through sensory fibers contained in the nerves to the muscles. In the skin itself there are present two systems of sensory fibers which regenerate at different times in a nerve that has been severed, and may be studied separately by this means. One system convex- sensations of pain and of extreme changes in tempera- ture, but the sensations are imperfectly localized and the sensi- bility is low, or, to express the same idea in another way. the threshold is high. This kind of sensation is found in the viscera also, and it may be considered from the functional standpoint as a defensive agency toward pathological changes in the tissues; it is designated as protopathic sensibility. It is stated that the glans penis possesses only this kind of sensibility. Protopathic sensibility comprises three qualities of sensation and presum- ably three sets of nerve-fibers, namely — for pain, for heat (not stimulated below 37° C), and for cold (not stimulated above 26° C). The second system of fibers responds to stimulations by light pressures and small differences in temperature between 26° and 37° C, the range of temperature to which the tem- perature nerves of the protopathic system are insensitive. These fibers regenerate after lesions much e slowly than the pro- topathic variety, and since the sensations mediated by them are localized very exactly, they furnish us the means for making fine discriminations of touch and temperature. For this reason they arc described us ;,n epicritic system, and the corresponding sensations arc designated as epicritic sensibility. This system of fibers is not found in the other organs, and it constitutes, therefore, the special characteristic of the skin area. In this ■ in there are included separate libers for heat, for cold, for tactile localization, and lor tactile discrimination. It is through the sensations mediated by these libers thai we recognize the shape and size of object s. The Punctiform Distribution of the Cutaneous Senses. A mo-t interesting fact in regard to the cutaneous senses is that they arc qoI distributed uniformly over the whole skin, but are it in discrete points or spots. This fad was first clearly established by Blix,* although it was discovered independently by Goldscheider and in this country by Donaldson. These ob- - paid attention chiefly to the Warm and cold spots. The Zeitschrifl f. Bioloeie," 20, III, 1884; Donaldson, "Mind," 39, I, 1886. See also Goldscheider, ?'Archivf. Physiologie, " 1885, suppl. volume. CUTANEOUS AND INTERNAL SENSATIONS. 273 existence of these spots may be demonstrated easily by anyone upon himself by moving a metallic point gently over the skin. If the point has a temperature below that of the skin it will be noticed that at certain spots it arouses simply a feeling of contact or pressure, while at other spots it gives a distinct sensation of coldness. If, on the other hand, the point is warmer than the skin it will at certain spots give a sensation of warmth. On mark- ing the cold and warm spots thus obtained it is found that they •• * Fig. 118. — Representation of the distribution of cold and warm spots on the volar surface of forearm in a space 2 cms. by 4 cms. The red dots represent the cold spots as tested at a temperature of 10° C. The black dots represent the warm spots as tested at a temperature of 41° to 48° C. occupy different positions on the skin. Elaborate charts have been made of the warm and cold spots on different regions of the skin, the apparatus usually employed being a metal tube through which water of any desired temperature may be circulated. The temperature of the skin, whatever it may be, forms the zero line; any object of a higher temperature stimulates only the warm spots, while one of a lower temperature acts upon the cold spots. The pressure or tactile sense and the pain sense are also distributed in a punctiform manner; they have been studied most carefully by von Frey.* To determine the loca- * Von Frey, " Konigl. Sachsischen Gesellschaft der Wissenchaften. Math.- phys. Klasse," 1894-95-96. IS 274 THE SPECIAL SENSES. tion of the pressure points he used fine hairs of different diam- eters fastened to a wooden handle. The cross-areas of these hairs are determined by measurements under the micro- scope, and the pressure exerted by each is measured by pressing it upon the scale pan of a balance. The quotient of the pressure exerted divided by the cross-area of the hair in square millimeters, J^i2, reduces the pressure to a uniform unit of area. For the pain points fine needles may be employed or stiff hairs similar to those used for the pressure points. From the experiments made there seems to be no doubt that each of the four cutaneous senses has its own spots of distribution in the skin, those for pain being most numerous and those for warmth the least numerous. There is some reason for believing also that the nerve endings mediating the pain sense lie most superficially in the skin and those for the warm sense the deepest. Specific Nerve Energies of the Cutaneous Nerves. — Many attempts have been made to determine whether the doctrine of specific nerve energies applies to these cutaneous senses; that is, whether each sense has its own nerve fibers capable of giving only its own quality of sensation. The evidence, on the whole, is favorable to this view. According to some observers, electrical or mechanical stimulation of the different points calls forth for each its character- istic reaction. Donaldson has found that cocain applied to the eye or throat destroys the senses of pain and pressure, but leaves those of heat and cold, which again supports the view of separate fibers for each sense. In addition there are a number of interesting pathological cases which point in the same direction. In some lesions of the cord — syringomyelia, for instance — the senses in the skin of the parts below are dissociated, — that is, there may be loss of pain and temperature in a certain area with :i retention of the pressure sense, — a fact which indicates that these senses have separate paths and therefore separate nerve-fibers.* Still more interesting eases of dissociation are reported as the result of the compression of peripheral nerve trunks. Tims, liarkerf describes his own case, in which, as the result, of the pressure of a cervical rib upon some of the cords of the brachial plexus, there was a region in the arm lacking iii t he pressure and temperature senses, but retain- ing the sense of pain. He quotes other cases in which the reverse dissociation occurred, pressure sense alone remaining. The simplest explanation Of these facts is the view that each pressure, pain, .\ .iciii. and eold spot, is supplied by its own nerve fiber, and that each, when stimulated, reacts, if it reacts at all, only with its own peculiar quality of sensation. According to this view, artificial * I'm- many interesting cases of dissociation due i«> spinal lesions, see Head, ' Brain/' L906, 537. | Barker, "Journal of Experimental Medicine," 1,348, L896. CUTANEOUS AND INTERNAL SENSATIONS. 275 stimulation, if properly controlled, of the trunks of the nerves supplying the skin should be capable of bringing out these different sense qualities. Experiments made with this point in view have not, however, been very successful. Mechanical or electrical stimu- lation of the ulnar nerve, for instance, gives usually only pain sensa- tions, although if the stimulus is feeble contact sensations are aroused. The method, however, is probably at fault. In the case of amputated fingers or limbs a more decisive result is obtained. As is well known, individuals after such operations may for many years have sensations of their lost fingers or limbs. In such cases the pressure in the stump of the wound acting upon the central ends of the sensory fibers arouses sensations which are projected in the usual way, and give the feeling that would be experienced if the lost parts were still there and were stimulated in the normal manner. The Temperature Senses. — The main facts regarding the distribution of heat and cold spots have been determined, but in most of the experiments on record no distinction was made between protopathic temperature sensations and those mediated by the epicritic temperature nerves. It is difficult to adapt the older descriptions to this newer terminology, but when not otherwise specifically stated it may be assumed that the epicritic system is referred to. In general, the cold spots are more numerous than the warm spots, and react more promptly to their adequate stimulus. The threshold stimulus varies in different parts of the skin, the tip of the tongue requiring the smallest stimulus to arouse a sensation, and the eyelids, fore- head, cheeks, lips, limbs, and trunk following in the order named. According to Goldscheider, the spots on most portions of the skin form chains that have a somewhat radiate arrange- ment with reference to the hair follicles. The temperature points possess each its adequate stimulus, that for the cold spot being temperatures lower than the skin or of the terminal organ of the cold nerves, that for the heat spots temperatures higher than their own. Apparently, therefore, one end-organ is excited by a diminution in the atomic movements of its organ, and the other by an increase. Nothing is known, however, of the exact nature of the stimulating process. From the standpoint of specific nerve energies it is most interesting to find that these points, particu- larly the cold spots, may be stimulated by other than their adequate stimuli. Mechanical and electrical stimulation has in the hands of several observers been efficient in causing a sensation of cold upon a cold spot and of heat upon a warm spot. Some chemical stimuli are also effective. Menthol applied to the skin gives a cold sensa- tion, while, on the other hand, if the arm be plunged into a jar of 276 THE SPECIAL SENSES. carbon-dibxid gas a distinct warm sensation will be experienced. A curious effect of this kind is what is known as the paradoxical cold reaction. It is produced by applying a very warm object, with a temperature of 40° to 60° C, to a cold spot. According to Head and Rivers this reaction is rather characteristic of the protopathic temperature fibers. It can be obtained, for example, from the glans penis, which possesses only protopathic sensibility, or during the course of regeneration of a severed cutaneous nerve. In this latter condition hot objects applied to a cold spot give a vivid sensation of cold. The same result may l>e felt sometimes at the instant of entering a hot bath. Many efforts have been made to determine whether there is a specific kind of end-organ for each of these senses. Numerous observers have cut out the skin from cold or hot spots and examined the removed part carefully by histological methods. The general result has been that no distinctive end-organs have been found. Von Frey, however, believes that, although the heat spots are supplied simply by a terminal end plexus, the cold spots in some places at least have as a special end-organ the end-bulbs of Krause. This conclusion is based upon the fact that these end-bulbs are found in places, such as the glans penis and con- junctiva, where the cold sense is especially prominent or exclu- sively present. The (Epicritic) Sense of Pressure or Touch. — The cutaneous pressure points are smaller and more numerous than the cold or warm spots. Von Frey has shown that in those portions of the body that are supplied with hairs the pressure points lie over the hair follicles. The pressure nerve-fibers, in fact. terminate in a ring surrounding the hair follicle, this form of termination serving as an end-organ. On account of their position they are stimulated by any pressure exerted upon the hair. The haii', indeed, acts like a lever and transmits any pres- sure applied to if with increased intensity, acting, therefore, as re- gards the pre—lire organ somewhat: like the eardxines in the case of the endings of the auditory nerve. In parts of the body nol furnished with hairs the tactile or Meissner corpuscles are found and these structures doubtless function as pressure end-organs. They are particularly abundant in the parts of the hand and feel in whicha delicate sense of pressureis present inspiteof a much thick- ened epidermis. It has been estimated that for the entire surface of the body, excluding the head region, there are about: 500,000 of the i pre ure point.-'. These points are close together on those parts, such as the tongue and fingers, which have a delicate tactile and more widely scattered where the sense is less developed. The Threshold Stimulus and the Localizing Power.- The delicacy of the cue of pressure may be measured by determining CUTANEOUS AND INTERNAL SENSATIONS. 277 the minimal pressure necessary to arouse a sensation, — that is, the threshold stimulus, — or it may be estimated in terms of the power of discriminating two contiguous stimuli, — that is, the mini- mal distance that two points must be apart in order for the sensa- tions to be recognized as distinct. The two methods of measure- ment do not coincide. As determined by the threshold stimulus, the greatest delicacy is exhibited by the skin of the face, the fore- head, and temples. According to the older methods of measure- ment, the forehead will perceive a pressure of 2 mgs., while the skin of the tips of the fingers needs a pressure of from 5 to 15 mgs. to arouse a perceptible sensation. The back of the hand or the arm is more sensitive from this standpoint than the tips of the fingers. When measured by the power of discriminating two points — that is, the localizing sense — the tips of the fingers are far more sensitive than the skin of the face or of the arm. This latter prop- erty, in fact, stands in relation to the closeness of the pressure points to one another. The localizing sense may be determined by Weber's method of using a pair of compasses with blunt points. For any given area of the skin the power of discrimination or local- ization is expressed in terms of the number of millimeters between the two points at which they are just distinguished as two separate sensations when applied simultaneously to the skin. Instruments made for this purpose are designated as esthesiometers. They carry two points the distance of which apart can be readily adjusted and read off on a scale. The most satisfactory form of esthesiom- eter is that devised by von Frey. The two points in this case are made by long, rather stiff hairs whose pressure can be made quite uniform. According to the older measurements, the localizing sense of different parts of the skin varies greatly, as is shown by the accompanying table: Tip of the tongue 1.1 mms. Tip of finger, palmar surface 2.3 " Second phalanx finger, palmar surface 4.5 " First phalanx finger, palmar surface 5.5 " Third phalanx finger, dorsal surface 6.8 " Middle of palm S to 9 " Second phalanx finger, dorsal surface 11.3 " Forehead 22.6 " Back of the hand 31.6 " Forearm 40.6 " Sternum 45 Along the spine 54 " Middle of neck or back 67.7 " The tips of the tongue and the fingers are, therefore, the most delicate surfaces, and that the tongue surpasses the fingers in this respect is easily within the experience of everyone who will recall the ease with which small objects between the teeth are detected by 27S THE SPECIAL SENSES. the tongue as compared with the fingers. From the above data it is evident also that the whole skin may be imagined as composed of a mosaic of areas of different sizes, the sensory circles of Weber, in each of which two or more simultaneous stimulations of the pres- sure nerves give only one pressure sensation. The size of these areas, particularly where they are large, may be reduced by practice, as is shown by the increased tactile sensibility of the blind. The fact that we can recognize two simultaneous pressure stimuli of the skin as two distinct sensations implies that the two sensations have some recognizable difference in consciousness. This difference is spoken of as the local sign. We may believe that every sensitive point upon the skin has its own distinctive local sign or quality, and that by experience we have learned to project each local sign more or less accurately to its proper place on the skin surface. Two points on this surface that are a great distance apart are easily recognized as different; but as we bring the points closer together the difference becomes less marked and finally disappears when the distance corresponds to the area of the sensory circle for the part of the skin investigated, for instance, 1 mm. for the tongue, 22 mms. for the forehead, etc. The ultimate limit of the power of discrimination was assumed by Weber to depend upon the area of distribution of a single nerve fiber. Assuming that each nerve fiber at its termi- nation spreads over a certain skin area, it was suggested that the size of this area forms a limit to the power of discrimination, since two stimuli within it would affect a single fiber and therefore would give a single sensation. This view, however, has not been supposed to accord with the facts even when the additional supposition was made that the local signs of two adjacent fibers may not be distinct enough for us to recognize them as separate and that practically there must be a number of intervening unstimulated areas, the number varying according to the sensitiveness of the area. Von Frey has, however, given a new method of testing the localizing sense of the skin, the results of which seem to accord with this anatomical explanation. [f instead of applying the two points simultaneously they are applied in succession, at an interval of one second, t he individual can distinguish the difference when two neighboring pressure points are stimulated. Each pressure point in the skin, therefore, has a local sign, which enables us to distinguish it from all others, and by this method the ultimate sensor} circles on the skin become much raialler than when measured by the usual method of Weber. The center of each is a pressure point and the area is determined by the distance from this center at which an isolated stimulation of this poinl can be obtained, ft seems probable, moreover, that each of these pressure points is connected to the brain byaseparate nerve path, possibly a single fiber, and thai this anatomical arrangement CUTANEOUS AND INTERNAL SENSATIONS. 279 determines the limitation of the localizing sense for different regions of the skin. In the newer work of Head and Rivers, which has been referred to several times, it will be recalled that they distinguish first of all between cutaneous sensibility to pressure and a deep sense of pressure. When the cutaneous fibers of a given area are all destroyed by degeneration, the area is still sen- sitive to pressure applied so as to deform the skin inward. The spot so stimulated can be localized accurately. This deep sense of pressure is mediated by the deep nerve fibers which supply the muscles. According to these authors the cutaneous pressure sensibility is mediated by two sets of fibers, those which give us the power of tactile discrimination when the compass points are applied simultaneously to the skin, and those which give us the power of tactile localization simply. In lesions of the spinal cord one of these sensibilities may be lost and the other retained over a given skin area. (Head and Thompson, "Brain," 1906.) In fact, the fibers of tactile discrimi- nation are stated to pass up the cord (uncrossed) in the posterior funiculi, while those of tactile localization ascend in the lateral or anterolateral funiculi and cross before reaching the medulla. The Pain Sense. — Pain is probably the sense that is most widely distributed in the body. It is present throughout the skin, and under certain conditions may be aroused by stimulation of sensory nerves in the various visceral organs, and indeed in all of the mem- branes of the body. Our knowledge of the physiological properties of the end-organs and nerves mediating this sense is chiefly limited to the skin, and for cutaneous pain at least the evidence, as stated above, is very strongly in favor of the view that there exists a special set of fibers which have a specific energy for pain. All recent ob- servers agree that the pain sense has a punctiform distribution in the skin, the pain points being even more numerous than the pres- sure points. The threshold stimulus of these points in various regions may be determined by von Frey's stimulating hairs, and experiments of this kind show, as we should expect, that it varies greatly. The cornea, for instance, gives sensations of pain with much weaker stimuli than in the case of the finger tips. In general, however, the threshold stimulus is much higher for the pain than for the pressure points. Histological examination of the pain points indicates that there is no special end-organ, the stimulus taking effect upon the free endings of the nerve fibers. Any of the usual forms of artificial nerve stimuli may affect these endings if of suf- ficient intensity, and, as is well known, stimuli applied to sensory nerve trunks affect these fibers with especial ease. A temperature of 50° to 70° C. applied to an afferent nerve will cause violent pain sensations, but has no effect upon the motor nerve fibers in the same trunk. Mechanical stimulation gives usually only pain sensations, and the results of inflammatory changes, as in neuritis or neuralgia, are equally marked. Localization or Projection of Pain Sensations. — Under normal conditions cutaneous pains are projected with accuracy to the point stimulated, and it is possible that this result is due in part at least 280 THE SPECIAL SENSES. to the training acquired in connection with concomitant (epicritic) pressure sensations, the latter acting as a guide or aid in the pro- jection. Thus in the cases referred to above, in which a portion of the skin had lost the sense of pressure and temperature, but retained that of pain, it was found that the localization was very incomplete. Pain arising in the internal organs, on the contrary, is located very inaccurately. The pain from a severe toothache, for example, may be projected quite diffusely to the side of the face. A very interesting fact in this connection is that such pains are often referred to points on the skin and may be accompanied by skin areas of tenderness. Pains of this kind that are misreferred to the surface of the body are designated as reflected pains. It has been shown by Head * and others that the different visceral organs have, in this respect, a more or less definite relation to certain areas of the skin. Pains arising from stimuli acting upon the intestines are located in the skin of the back, loins, and abdomen in the area supplied by the ninth, tenth, and eleventh dorsal spinal nerves; pains from irritations in the stomach are located in the skin over the ensiform cartilage; those from the heart in the scapular region, and so on. The explanation offered for this misreference is that the pain is referred to the skin region that is supplied from the spinal segment from which the organ in question receives its sensory fibers, the misreference being due to a diffusion in the nerve centers. As Head expresses it, "when a painful stimulus is applied to a part of low sensibility in close central connection with a part of much greater sensibility the pain pro- duced is felt in the part of higher sensibility rather than in the part of lower sensibility to which the stimulus was actually applied." It is interesting that affections of the serous cavities — c. g., the peritoneum— do not cause reflected pains or cutaneous tenderness as in the case of the viscera. Another notable fact in this connec- tion is the occurrence of the condition known as allochiria. When from any cause one or other of the cutaneous senses is depressed in a given area stimulation in this region may give sensations which are referred to the symmetrical area on the other side of 1 1n- body, or, it this also is involved, it may be referred to the area 1 1 < ■ . i above or below in the spinal order. The above law, according to which projection is made to the area of high sensi- bility most, closely connected with the area of low sensibility, .-(.•in- to hold in this case also. Muscular or Deep Sensibility. The existence of a special set of sensory nerve-fibers distributed to the muscles was clearly gnized by some of the older physiologists. Charles Bell, f Head, Brain," Hi, I, 1893, and 24, 345, L901. i Bell, 'lli'- Nervous System of the Human Body," third edition, Lon- don, isi 1, p. 200 CUTANEOUS AND INTERNAL SENSATIONS. 281 for example, says: "Between the brain and the muscles there is a circle of nerves; one nerve conveys the influence from the brain to the muscle; another gives the sense of the condition of the muscle to the brain." The conclusive proof of the existence of such fibers, however, has only been fur- nished within recent years. It has been demonstrated that there are special sensory endings in the muscles, the so-called muscle spindles, and in the attached tendons, the tendon spindles or tendon organs of Golgi. The muscle spindles are found most frequently in the neighborhood of the tendons, at tendinous inter- sections or under aponeuroses. Sherrington* has shown that the nerve fibers in them do not degenerate after section of the anterior roots of the corresponding spinal nerves and are therefore derived from the posterior roots. In the muscles of the limbs he estimates that from one-half to one-third of the fibers in the muscular nerve branches are sensory, and that most of these sensory fibers end in the muscle spindles. On the physiological and clinical side facts of various kinds have accumulated that make clear the existence of this group of sensory fibers and emphasize their essential importance in the co-ordination of our muscular movements. It has been shown that stimulation of the nerves distributed to the muscles or mechani- cal stimulation of the muscles themselves causes a depressor effect upon blood-pressure, thus demonstrating the presence of afferent fibers in the muscles. As described in the section upon the central nervous system, the numerous experiments upon the effect of section of the posterior and lateral funiculi of the cord, and observations upon the results of pathological lesions of the posterior funiculi (tabes dorsalis) give results which are interpreted to mean that fibers of muscular sensibility form the most important group in the posterior funiculi and constitute, as well, perhaps, the long, ascending fibers in the cerebellospinal fasciculus in the lateral funiculi. It is believed, therefore, that our so-called voluntary muscles are richly supplied with afferent fibers and that the im- pulses carried by these fibers to the brain are necessary for the proper contraction of the muscles, and particularly for the ade- quate combination of the contractions of groups of muscles in the co-ordinated movements of equilibrium. Indeed, section of the posterior roots of the spinal nerves supplying a given region is followed by a loss of control of the muscles in this region hardly less complete than the paralysis produced by direct section of the anterior roots; the muscles not only lose their tonicity in consequence of the dropping out of the reflex sensory stimuli from the skin and muscles of the region, but they are apparently withdrawn from voluntary control in spite of the * Sherrington, "Journal of Physiology," 17, 237, 1894. 282 THE SPECIAL SENSES. maintenance of their normal motor connections. Within the central nervous system the fibers of muscle sense end in part in the cerebellum and in part pass forward, by way of the median fillet, to end in the cerebrum. In the cerebrum they end in the cortex of the parietal lobe in the region of the posterior central convolution. There is reason to believe that this cortical sense area of the muscle sense is connected by association fibers with the motor areas lying anterior to the central fissure of Rolando, and we have thus a reflex arc — or, as Bell expressed it, a circle of nerves between the muscles and the brain. It is probable thai a similar arc or circle is formed by the connections through the cerebellum, and still a third one of a lower order by the connections in the spinal cord. In the higher animals the impulses received in the cerebellum through the fibers of muscle sense, in connection with those received from the semicircular canals and vestibular sacs of the ear, furnish the sensory basis for the cerebellar control of muscular movements, particularly of the synergetic combinations necessary in locomotion. Through the circle or arc in the cortex of the cerebrum it may be supposed that our characteristic voluntary movements are affected, and it may be doubted whether a so-called voluntary contraction can be made when this circle is broken on the sensory side. Whether or not this latter suggestion is true, it seems to be beyond doubt that adequately controlled voluntary movements depend for their adaptation upon the inflow of sensory impulses along the fibers of muscle sense. We have a certain conscious- ness of the condition of our muscles at all times, and if we were deprived of this knowledge we should be unable to control them properly, perhaps unable to use them voluntarily. The Quality of the Muscular Sensibility. -Under the term muscular sensibility in its wide sense we must understand the aensibility mediated by the afferent fibers from the muscles, the tendons, ligaments, and joints. The quality of these deep sensations is of Beveral kinds we have first. of all the deep ure sensibility (see p. '-'7!)), which gives a definite conscious reaction thai is well Localized. It- is usually projected to the exterior and is ao1 consciously separated from the tactile or lie sensations of the skin. We probably make much use of this sensibility in judging the weight and resistance of bodies. Mu cular sensibility proper is that ill-defined consciousness which we possess of the condition and position of our muscles or of the joints or Limbs moved by them. It, includes also the 3en <■ of passive posh ion, and I he sense of effort and of the spatial relations of t he Limbs in mo1 ion or at rest. The untrained person scarcely recognizes the exi bence of these sensations, but they CUTANEOUS AND INTERNAL SENSATIONS. 283 are evident enough upon analysis, and it is most certain that they take a fundamental part in regulating our movements. In all our estimations of the extent of the muscular contractions they form the chief sensory basis, and in this way they may indirectly furnish us with data for perceptions and judgments of various kinds. Thus, in the judgments of distance based upon visual impressions it is believed that for close objects, particularly, the muscle sense connected with the extrinsic and intrinsic musculature of the eyeballs plays a fundamental part. Doubtless also this sense takes an essential part in the primitive formation of our conceptions of space, since it may be assumed that the continual movements of the extremities in connection with our visual and tactile impressions furnish essential data upon which we build our perceptions of distance and size, our judgments of spatial relations. As is explained in the chapter on the Physiology of the Ear, the sensations from the semi- circular canals and vestibular sacs co-operate in giving data for these fundamental conceptions, and it is not possible for us to disentangle the parts taken by these senses separately in building up our knowledge of the external world. In excessive muscular effort the quality of the muscle sensation undergoes a change and becomes strong enough to make a distinct and peculiar impression upon our consciousness. We designate this feeling as fatigue, but there is no question apparently that this sensation is mediated through the same nerve-fibers that ordinarily give us our muscular sensibility. Sensations of Hunger and Thirst. — Hunger and thirst are typical interior (or common) sensations. We feel them as changes in ourselves. Neither sense has been the direct object of much experimental investigation, and what knowledge we possess is there- fore derived largely from accidental or pathological sources. Hunger in its mild form is designated as appetite. It occurs normally at a certain interval after meals, and is referred or projected more or less accurately to the stomach. It is not known whether this sense is mediated by a special set of sensory fibers distributed to the mucous membrane of the stomach, or whether, perhaps, it may be a quality of the sensory impressions from the muscular coat. The former view seems more probable, especially when it is remembered that loss of appetite or anorexia is so frequently an accompaniment of pathological changes in the membrane of the stomach. The nervous mechanism through which this sense is me- diated is of most essential importance and deserves more careful study at the hands of physiologists and pathologists. Under ordi- nary conditions of life all of the regulation of the amount and quality of the food necessary to the proper nutrition of the body and the 2S4 THE SPECIAL SENSES. maintenance of body equilibrium is effected through this sense. Its striking influence upon the body at large is well illustrated in the case of animals (pigeons, dogs) deprived of their cerebrum. During the period of fasting these animals show all the external signs of hunger and keep in continual, restless movement that seems to imply a con- stantly acting sensory stimulus. We may assume that appetite has its sensory origin, its peripheral nerve endings in the stomach, and that these endings are excited in some unknown way when the stomach is empty. This gastric hunger, as it might be called, disappears, or the appetite is appeased when the stomach is filled. This fact in itself would indicate that the stimulus has a local origin in the stomach, and is not dependent upon any general change in the nutritive condition of the body. The appetite is satisfied by filling the stomach with food long before this food is actually absorbed and distributed to the tissues. The inges- tion of totally indigestible material would probably have temporarily a similar result. The exact nature of the conditions that lead to or cause a stimulation of the sensory nerves of appetite in the stomach remains unexplained. The well-known fact that muscular exercise and low temperatures and particularly a combination of the two cause a marked augmentation of the appetite would suggest that the sensory stimulus is influenced by the extent or character of the oxidations in the muscular tissues, and that, therefore, some substance may be formed as the result of these oxidations which affects the sensory nerves of the stomach. The same general sug- gestion is contained in the fact that diabetics exhibit an abnormal appetite in spite of abundant feeding. In these individuals the carbohydrate food escapes oxidation more or less completely, and the metabolism, particularly in the muscles, involves, therefore, to a greater exlenl , the oxidation of protein material, — a fact which may stand iii some relation to the abnormal appetite thai .is observed. The complexity of the nervous apparatus that controls I he 1 he appe- tite is shown also by many facts from the experiences of life and from the results of laboratory investigations. For example, it is found that large amounts of gelatin in the diet, although at. first, accepted willingly, soon provoke a feeling of dislike and aversion to this particular foodstuff such as cannot be overcome. All animal will Btarve rather than use the gelatin, although all of our direct, physio- logical evidence would indicate that this substance is an efficient food, playing much the same part as the fats or carbohydrates. A fact of t his kind indicates that the sensory apparal us of the appe- tite is influenced in some specific way by the metabolism of this particular material. So also the feeling of satiety and aversion for food i hat follows overfeeding indicates something more than a sim- ple removal of the sensations of appetite; it implies an active state, CUTANEOUS AND INTERNAL SENSATIONS. 285 due possibly to the excitation of sensory fibers of a different char- acter. With regard to the effects of prolonged starvation, the pangs of hunger that are felt at first do not seem to increase in in- tensity to such an extent as to cause actual suffering. The testi- mony of the "professional f asters," at least, seems to show that, if water is provided, prolonged deprivation of food is not accompanied by the intense discomfort or suffering popularly associated with the idea of complete starvation. The Sense of Thirst. — Our sensations of thirst are projected more or less accurately to the pharynx, and the facts that we know would seem to indicate that the sensory nerves of this region have the important function of mediating this sense. The water con- tents of the body are subject to great changes. Through the lungs, the skin, and the kidneys water is lost continually in amounts that vary with the conditions of life. This loss affects the blood directly, but is doubtless made good, so far as this tissue is concerned, by a call upon the great mass of water contained in the storehouse of the tissues. To restore the body tissues to their normal equilibrium in water we ingest large quantities, and the control of this regula- tion is effected through the sense of thirst. We know little or nothing about the nervous apparatus involved; but it may be assumed that when the water content falls below a certain amount the nerve fibers in the pharyngeal membrane (fibers of the glosso- pharyngeal nerve) are stimulated and give us the sensation of thirst. That we have in this membrane a special end-organ of thirst is indicated, moreover, by the fact that local drying in this region, from dry or salty food, or dry and dusty air, produces a sensation of thirst that may be appeased by moistening the mem- brane with a small amount of water not in itself sufficient to relieve a genuine water need of the body. Our normal thirst sensations might be designated, therefore, as pharyngeal thirst, to indicate the probable origin of the sensory stimuli. Prolonged deprivation of water, however, must affect the water content of all the tissues, and under these conditions sensations are experienced whose quality is not that of simple thirst alone, but of pain or suffering. All ac- counts agree that complete deprivation of water for long periods induces intense discomfort, anguish, and possibly mental troubles, and we may suppose that under these conditions sensory nerves are stimulated in many tissues, and that the metabolism in the ner- vous system in addition is directly affected by the loss of water. It is interesting to note that, while in diseases due to a general in- fection, loss of appetite, anorexia, is a frequent symptom, there is no corresponding loss of the sense of thirst. Even in hydrophobia the patient experiences the sensations of thirst, although unable to drink water. CHAPTER XVI. SENSATIONS OF TASTE AND SMELL. The sense of taste is mediated by nerve fibers distributed to parts of the buccal cavity and particularly to parts of the tongue. The most sensitive regions are the tip, the borders, and the posterior portion of the dorsum of the tongue in the region of the circum- vallate papillae. Taste buds and a sense of taste are described also for the soft palate, the epiglottis, and even for the larynx. The sense is not present uniformly over the entire dorsum of the tongue. On the contrary, it has an irregular, punctiform distribution over most of this region with the exception of the parts mentioned above. The Nerves of Taste. — The anterior two-thirds of the tongue are supplied with sensory fibers from the lingual nerve, a branch of the inferior maxillary division of the fifth nerve, and the posterior third from the glossopharyngeal. The taste fibers for these regions, therefore, are supplied immediately through these nerves. It has been shown, moreover, that the taste fibers carried in the lingual are brought to it through the chorda tympani nerve, which arises from the seventh cranial nerve and joins the lingual soon after emerging from the tympanic cavity of the ear. There has been much discussion as to the origin of these taste fibers from the brain. At first si <• considered as a combination of ;i salty taste with common sensibility. Combinations of sweel and acid tastes, sweet, Fig. 120. — Section through one of the taste of i Ik- papilla foliata of the rabbit (from Quoin, after Ranvier), highly magnified: p, Gus- tatory pore: e, gustatory cell; r, mstentacular cell; »(. leucocyte containing granules; e, super- ficial epithelial cell ; /», nerve fibei SENSATIONS OF TASTE AND SMELL. 289 and bitter tastes, etc., form a part of our daily experience, and in the fused or compound sensation that results from such com- binations one may usually recognize without difficulty the con- stituent parts. The seemingly great variety of our taste sensations is largely due to the fact that we confuse them or combine them with simultaneous odor sensations. Thus, the flavors in fruits and the bouquet of wines are due to odor sensations which we designate ordinarily as tastes, since they are experienced at the time these objects are ingested. If care is taken to shut off the nasal cavities during the act of ingestion even imperfectly, as by holding the nose, the so-called taste disappears in large measure. Very dis- agreeable tastes are usually, as a matter of fact, due to unpleasant odor sensations. On the other hand, some volatile substances which enter the mouth through the nostrils and stimulate the taste organs are interpreted by us as odors. The odor of chloro- form, for instance, is largely due to stimulation of the sweet taste in the tongue. Distribution and Specific Energy of the Fundamental Taste Sensations. — Regarding the distribution of the funda- mental taste sensations over the tongue and palate there seem to be many individual differences. In general, however, it may be said that the bitter taste is more developed at the back of the tongue and the adjacent or posterior regions; at the tip of the tongue the bitter sense is less marked or in cases may be absent altogether. On the contrary, in this latter region the sweet taste is well developed. On this account it may happen that substances which when first taken into the mouth give a not unpleasant sweet taste subsequently when swallowed cause disagreeably bitter sen- sations, like the little book of the evangelist, which in the mouth was "sweet as honey, and as soon as I had eaten it. my belly was bitter." Oehrwall* has made an interesting series of experiments in which he stimulated separately a number of fungiform papillae on the surface of the tongue. Each papilla was stimulated sepa- rately for its fundamental taste senses of sweet, bitter, and acid, by using drops of solutions of sugar, quinin, and tartaric acid. Of the 125 papillse thus examined, 27 gave no reaction at all, although sensitive to pressure and temperature. In the 98 papilla? that reacted to the sapid stimulation it was found that 60 gave taste sensations of all three qualities, 4 gave only sweet and bitter, 7 only bitter and acid, 12 only sweet and acid, 12 only acid, and 3 only sweet. None was found to give only a bitter sensation. These facts bear directly upon the question of the specific energy of the taste fibers. It is possible that the four fundamental taste qualities may be mediated by four different end-organs and four separate * Oehrwal], "Skandinavisches Archiv f. Physiologie," 2, 1, 1S90. 19 290 THE SPECIAL SENSES. sets of nerve fibers, each giving, when stimulated, only its own quality of sensation. On the other hand, it is possible that one and the same nerve fiber might give different qualities of sensation according to the nature and mode of action of the sapid substances. The fact, as shown by Oehrwall's experiments, that there are sensory spots upon the tongue which will not react to some kinds of sapid substance, but do react to others, and perhaps only to one particular kind, speaks strongly in favor of the view that there are different end-organs and nerve fibers for each fundamental taste. This view is still further supported by the fact that certain chemically pure sub- stance : give different tastes according to the part of the tongue upon which the}' are placed. Thus, sodium sulphate (Guyot) may taste salty upon the tip of the tongue and bitter when placed upon the posterior part. A better instance still is given by solutions of a bromin substitution product of saccharin, the chemical name f CO for which is parabrom-benzoic sulphinid: C6H3Br 1 ~*~ ^NH. When this substance is placed upon the tip of the tongue it gives a sweet sensation, while upon the posterior region it gives only a bitter taste together with a sensation of astringency (Howell and Kastle). Extracts of the leaves of a tropical plant, Gymnema silvestre, applied to the tongue, destroy the sense of taste for sweet and bitter sub- stances (Shore), and this fact may be explained most satisfactorily by assuming that this substance exercises a selective action upon taste terminals in the tongue, paralyzing those for the bitter and the sweet substances. Finally, the fact that electrical, me- chanical, or chemical stimulation of the chorda tympani, where it passes through the tympanic cavity, may arouse taste sensations is proof that the taste sensation in general is not due to a peculiar kind of impulse that can be aroused only by the action of sapid bodies upon the terminals in the tongue, but, on the contrary, that it is a specific energy of these fibers, and depends for its quality, there- fore, upon the specific reaction of the terminations in the brain. Method of Sapid Stimulation. — In order that sapid substances may react upon the taste terminals il is necessary, in the first place, thai they shall be in solution. It is impossible to taste with a dry tongue. We may a aume, therefore, that the stimulation consists c < m i.'illy in :i chemical reaction between the sapid substance and the terminal of the taste fiber, — for instance, the hair process of the sense cells in the taste buds, and the question naturally arises whether the distinctive reactions corresponding to the separate qualities can be referred to a definite chemical structure in the Bapid bodi( , Are there certain chemical groups which posses:: I he property of reacting specifically with the end-organs? Experience show- that substances of very different chemical const it ulion may SENSATIONS OF TASTE AND SMELL. 291 excite the same taste. Thus, sugar, saccharin, and sugar of lead (lead acetate) all give a sweet taste, while, on the other hand, starch (soluble starch), which stands so close in structure to the sugars, has no effect upon the taste terminals. It is interesting to remember that the taste nerves may be stimulated by sapid sub- stances dissolved in the blood as well as when applied to the ex- terior of the tongue. A sweet taste maybe experienced in diabetes from the sugar in the blood, or a bitter taste in jaundice from the bile. The Threshold Stimulus. — The determination of the threshold stimulus for different sapid substances is made by ascertaining the minimal concentration of the solution which is capable of arousing a taste sensation. The delicacy of the sense of taste is influenced, however, by certain accessory conditions which must be taken into account. Thus, the temperature of the solution is an important condition. Very cold or very hot solutions do not react, — that is, the extremes of temperature seem to diminish or destroy the sensi- tiveness of the end-organ. A temperature between 10° and 30° C. gives the optimum reaction. So also the delicacy of the sense of taste is increased by rubbing the sapid solution against the tongue. Doubtless this mechanical action facilitates the penetration of the sapid body into the mucous membrane, but it seems also to in- crease the irritability of the end-organ. It is our habit in tasting bodies with the tongue to rub this organ against the hard palate. With regard to the threshold stimulus such results as the following are reported: Salty (sodium chlorid). 0.25 gm. in 100 c.c. H20 — detectible on tip of tongue. Sweet (sugar) 0.50 " " " " detectible on tip of tongue. Acid (HC1) 0.007 " " " " detectible on border of tongue. Bitter (quinin) 0.00005 " " " " detectible on root of tongue. The very great sensitiveness of the tongue to bitter substances is evident from this table : The Olfactory Organ. — The end-organ for the olfactory sense lies in the upper part of the nose, and consists of elongated, epithe- lial-like cells, each of which bears on its free end a tuft of six to eight hair-like processes, while at its basal end it is continued into a nerve fiber that passes through the cribriform plate of the ethmoid bone and ends in the olfactory bulb. These olfactory sense cells lie among supporting epithelial cells of a columnar shape (Fig. 121). At the free edge of the cells there is a limiting membrane through which the olfactory hairs project. The olfactor}r sense 292 THE SPECIAL SENSES. cells are essentially nerve cells, and in this respect resemble the sense cells in the retina, the rods and cones, rather than those of the ear or of the organs of taste. The distribution of the olfactory cells, according to v. Brunn, is confined to the nasal septum and a portion of the upper turbinate bone. The area covered in each nos- tril corresponds to about 250 square millimeters. The epithelium of the lower and middle turbinates and the floor of the nostrils is composed of the usual ciliated cells found in the respiratory passages, while the so-called vestibular region of the nose, the part roofed in by the cartilage, is covered hWr ky a stratified pavement epithelium corresponding in structure with that of the skin. These latter portions of the nose are supplied with sensory fibers derived from the fifth or trigeminal nerve. We must consider the 500 sq. mm. of olfac- tory epithelium as the olfactory sense organ com- parable physiologically and perhaps anatomically to the rod and cone layer of the retina. The connections of these cells with the central nervous system have al- ready been described (p. 213). It will be remem- bered that the fine, non- medullated fibers springing from the basal end of the sense cells enter the olfac- tory bulb and end in ter- minal arborizations in the olfactory glomeruli, where they make con- nections by contact with the dendrites of the mitral cells of the bulb. Through the axons of these mitral eells the impulses are con- ducted along < he olfactory tract to their various terminations in the Olfactory lobe itself, either of the same or of the opposite side, and nail, also in the cortical region, the uncinate gyrus, of the hippocampal lobe. As regards the olfactory sense cells, the nerve cells in the olfactory bulb niiydit, be compared with the nerve gan- glion layer of the retina, and the nerve libers of t he olfactory tract with the fibers of the optic nerve. The Mechanism of Smelling. — Odoriferous substances to Fig 121. Cells of the olfactory region (after r. Brunn): ", ", Olfactory cells; '», /<, epithelial eel] , //, a, central proce prolonged :< i an • .1 r .- 1 * ■ tory nerve fibril; I, I, nucleus; c, knob-like clear termination "i peripheral process; h, h, bunch of olfactory liair.s. SENSATIONS OF TASTE AND SMELL. 293 affect the olfactory cells must, of course, penetrate into the upper part of the nasal chamber. This end is attained during inspiration, either by simple diffusion or by currents produced by the act of sniffing. It may also happen by way of the posterior nares. In fact, the flavors of many foods, fruits, wine, etc., are olfactory rather than gustatory sensations. When such food is swallowed the poste- rior nares are shut off from the pharynx by the soft palate, but in the expiration succeeding the swallow the odor of the food is con- veyed to the olfactory end-organ. Flavors are perceived, therefore, not during the act of swallowing, but subsequently, and if the nostrils are blocked, as in coryza, foods lose much of their flavor. Simply holding the nose will destroy much of the so-called taste of fruits or the bouquet of wines.* Nature of the Olfactory Stimulus. — The fact that smells are transmitted through space like light and sound has suggested the possibility that they may depend upon a vibratory movement of some medium. This view, although occasionally defended in modern times, is apparently entirely incompatible with the facts. The usual view is that odoriferous bodies emit particles which, as a rule at least, are in gaseous form. These particles are con- veyed to the olfactory epithelium by currents in the air or by simple gaseous diffusion, and after solution in the moisture of the membrane act chemically upon the sensitive hairs of the sense cells. All vapors or gases are, however, not capable of acting as stimuli to these cells; so that evidently the odoriferous character depends upon some peculiarity of structure. It is assumed that there are certain groups, "odoriphore groups," which are character- istic of all odoriferous substances and by virtue of which these substances react with the special form of protoplasm found in the hair cells. Haycraftf has formulated certain fundamental conceptions bearing upon the relation between chemical structure and odoriferous stimulation. He has shown that the power to cause smell, like other physical properties, is a periodic function of the atomic weight— that in the periodic system, according to Men- dele jeff, the elements in certain groups are characterized by their odoriferous properties; for instance, the second, fourth, and sixth members — sulphur, selenium, and tellurium — of the sixth group. Moreover, in organic compounds belonging to an homologous series the smell gradually changes and, indeed, increases in the higher members of the series, — that is, in those having a more complex molecular structure. The Qualities of the Olfactory Sensations. — While we dis- * For many interesting facts concerning smelling and the literature to 1895 see Zwaardemaker, "Die Physiologie des Geruchs," Leipzig, 1895. tHaycraft, "Brain," 1888, p. 166. 294 THE SPECIAL SENSES. tingiiish a great many different kinds of odors, it has been found difficult, indeed impossible, to classify them very satisfactorily into groups. That is, it is not possible to pick out what might be called the fundamental odor sensations. This sense was doubtless used by primitive man chiefly in detecting and testing food, in protect- ing himself from noxious surroundings, and perhaps also in controll- ing his social relations. The olfactory sensations, in accordance with this use made of them, give either pleasant or unpleasant sensa- tions in a more marked and universal way than in the case of vision or hearing, approaching, in this respect, rather the purely sensual characteristics of the lower senses, the bodily appetites. Mankind has been content to classify odors as agreeable and disagreeable, and to designate the many different qualities of odors by the names of the substances which in his individual experience usually give rise to them. A number of observers have proposed classifications more or less complete in character. One of the latest and perhaps the best is that suggested by Zwaardemaker on the basis of the nomenclatures introduced by previous observers. Adopting first the general grouping into pure odors, odors mixed with sensa- tions of common sensibility from the mucous membrane of the nose, and odors mixed or confused with tastes, he separates the pure odors or odors proper into nine classes, as follows : I. Odores setherei or ethereal odors, such as are given by the fruits, which depend upon the presence of ethereal substances or esters. II. Odores aromatici or aromatic odors, which are typified by camphor and citron, bitter almond and the resinous bodies. This class is divided into five Bubgroups. III. Odores fragrantes, the fragrant or balsamic odors, comprising the vari- ous flower odors or perfumes. The class falls into three subgroups. IV. Odores ambrosiaci, the ambrosial odors, typified by amber and musk. This odor is present in the flesh, blood, Or excrement of some ani- mals, being referable in the I • t - 1 instance to the bile. V. Odores alliacei or garlic odors, such as are found in the onion, garlic, sulphur, selenium and lellurii jompounds. They fall into three subgroups. VI. Odores cnipvreumai ici or the burning odors, the odors given by roasted coffee, baked bread, tobacco smoke, etc. The odors of benzol, phenol, and the products of dry distillation of wood come into this class. VII. Odores bircmi or goal odors. The odor of this animal arises from the. caproic and caprylic acid contained in the sweat; cheese, sweat, spermatic and vaginal secretions give odors of a similar quality. VIII. Odores tetri or repulsive odors, such as are given by many of the nar- cotic plante and acani bus. IX. Odores nauseosi or nauseating or fetid odors, such as are given by feces and certain plant- and the products of putrefaction. While the classification serves to emphasize a number of marked resemblances or relations that exist, among the odors, it does not rest wholly upon a subjective kinship,— that is, the different odors brought together in one class do not in all cases arouse in us sensa- SENSATIONS OF TASTE AND SMELL. 295 tions that seem to be of related quality. It is not impossible, how- ever, that further analysis may succeed in showing that there are certain fundamental qualities in our numerous odor sensations. Our position regarding the odors is similar to that which formerly prevailed in the case of the taste sensations. It was thought to be impossible to classify these latter satisfactorily on the basis of a few fundamental sensations, but it is now universally accepted that all of our true gustatory sensations show one or more of four primary taste qualities. As was said above, our odor sensations are classi- fied in ordinary life as agreeable or disagreeable, and, indeed, Haller, the great physiologist of the eighteenth century, divided odors along this line into three classes: (1) the agreeable or am- brosial, (2) the disagreeable or fetid, and (3) the mixed odors. In many cases, no doubt, the agreeableness or disagreeableness of an odor depends solely upon the associations connected with it. If the associative memories aroused are unpleasant the odor is dis- agreeable. Thus, the odor of musk, so pleasant to most persons, produces most disagreeable sensations in others, on account of past associations. It is possible, however, that there is some funda- mental difference in physiological reaction between such odors as those of putrefaction and of a violet which may be considered as the cause of the difference in psychical effect. It has been suggested, for instance, that they may affect the circulation in the brain in opposite ways, one producing an increased, the other a decreased flow. This improbable supposition has been shown to be devoid of foun- dation by the observations of Shiel els.* In his experiments the vascu- lar supply to the skin of the arm was determined by plethysmo- graphic methods, and it was found that both pleasant (heliotrope perfume) and unpleasant (putrefactive) odors give a similar vascu- lar reaction. Each class, if it acts at all, causes, as a rule, a con- striction of the skin vessels, such as is obtained normally from in- creased mental activity, — a reaction usually interpreted to mean a greater flow of blood to the brain. Fatigue of the Olfactory Apparatus. — It is a matter of common observation that many odors, such as the perfumes of flowers, quickly cease to give a noticeable sensation when the stimu- lation is continued. This result is usually attributed to fatigue of the sense cells in the end-organ and it is noticeable chiefly with faint odors. One who sits in an ill-ventilated room occupied by many persons may be quite unconscious of the unpleasant odor from the vitiated air, while to a newcomer it is most distinct. Threshold Stimulus — Delicacy of the Olfactory Sense. — The extraordinary delicacy of the sense of smell in some of the lower animals is seemingly beyond the power of objective measurement or * Shields, " Journal of Experimental Medicine," 1, 1896. 296 THE SPECIAL SENSES. expression. The ability of a dog, for instance, to follow the trail of a given person depends undoubtedly upon the recognition of the individual odor, and the actual amount of olfactory material left upon the ground which serves as the stimulus must be infinitesi- mally small. Even in ourselves the actual amount of olfactory material which sui rices to give a distinct sensation is often beyond our means of determination except by the aid of calculation. It is recognized in chemical work, for instance, that traces of known substances too small to give the ordinary chemical reactions may be detected easily by the sense of smell. By taking known amounts Fif!. 122. — Zwaardemaker's olfactometer. of odoriferous substances and diluting them to known extents it is possible to express in weights the minimal amount of each substance th.it can cause a sensation. By this method such figures as the following are obtained: Camphor is perceived in a dilution of 1 part to I' 10,000; musk, 1 part to 8,000,000; vanillin, 1 part to 10,000,000; while, according to the experiments of Fischer and Penzoldt, mercaptan may be detected in a dilution of -jit 7nj*o"mr?r °f a milli- gram in 1 liter of air or -j-jnnrlnnnro" °f a milligram inSOc.c. of air. Various methods have been proposed to determine the relative delicacy <>f the olfactory sense in differenl persons, and these methods have some application in the clinical diagnosis of certain cases. Zwaardemaker has devised ;i simple apparatus, the olfactometer, the principle of which is illustrated in Fig. 122. It consists of an outside cylinder the olfactory cylinder whose inner surface is of porous material which can be filled with a known strength of olfac- tory solution and an inside tube, smelling tube. This latter is applied to the nose and where it runs inside the cylinder it is gradu- SENSATIONS OF TASTE AND SMELL. 297 ated in centimeters. It is evident that the further out the inner tube is pulled the greater will be the amount of olfactory substance which will be exposed to the incoming air of an inspiration. Conflict of Olfactory Sensations. — When different odors are inhaled simultaneously through the two nostrils they may give rise to the phenomenon of a conflict of the olfactory fields similar to that described for the visual fields. That is, we perceive first one then the other without obtaining a fused or compound sensation. The result depends largely on the odors selected. In some cases one odor may predominate in consciousness to the entire suppression of the other, — a phenomenon which also has an analogy in binocular sensations. It is well known, also, that certain odors antagonize or neutralize others. It is said, for instance, that the odor of iodoform, usually so persistent and so disagreeable, may be neutralized by the addition of Peru balsam, and that the odor of carbolic acid may destroy that of putrefactive processes. Whether the neutralization is of a chemical nature or is physiological does not seem to have been definitely ascertained. Olfactory Associations. — Personal experience shows clearly that olfactory sensations arouse numerous associations — our olfactory memories are good. On the anatomical side the cortical center in the hippocampal lobe is known to be widely connected with other parts of the cerebrum, and we have in this fact a basis for the extensive associations connected with odors. In animals like the dog, with highly developed olfactory organs, it is evident that this sense must play a correspondingly large part in the psychical life. In such animals as well as among the invertebrates it is in- timately connected with the sexual reflexes, and some remnant of this relationship is obvious among human beings. Among the so- called special senses that of smell is perhaps the one most closely connected with the bodily appetites, and overgratification or over- indulgence of this sense, according to historical evidence, has at least been associated with periods of marked decadence of virtue among civilized nations. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE EYE. The eye is the peripheral organ of vision. By means of its peculiar physical structure rays of light from external objects are focused upon the retina and there set up nerve impulses that are transmitted by the fibers of the optic nerve and optic tract to the visual center in the cortex of the brain. In this last organ is aroused that reaction in consciousness which we designate as a visual sensation. In studying the physiology of vision we may consider the eye. first, as an optical instrument physically adapted to form an image on the retina and provided with certain physi- ological mechanisms for its regulation; secondly, we may study the properties of the retina in relation to its reactions to light. and lastly, the visual sensations themselves, or the physiology of the visual center in the brain. CHAPTER XVII. THE EYE AS AN OPTICAL INSTRUMENT-DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. Formation of an Image by a Biconvex Lens. — That the re- fract ive surfaces of the eye form an image of external objects upon i In' retinal surface is a necessary conclusion from its physical struc- ture. The fact may be demonstrated directly, however, by ob- servation upon the excised eye of an albino rabbit. The thin coats of such an eye are semit ransparent , and if the eye is placed in a tube of blackened paper and held in front of one's own eyes it can be seen readily thai a small, inverted image of external objects is formed upon the retinal surface, just as an inverted image of the exterior is formed upon t he ground glass plate of a photographic camera. This image is formed in the eye by virtue of the refractive surfaces of the COrnea and t he lens. The curved surfaces of these t ransparent, bodies nbstanfially like ;i convex glass lens, and the physics of the formation of an image by such a lens may be used to explain the refractive processes in the eye. To understand the formation of. 'in image by a biconvex lens the following physical facts must he 298 DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 299 borne in mind. Parallel rays of light falling upon one surface of the lens are brought to a point or focus (F) behind the other surface (Fig. 123). This focus for parallel rays is the principal focus and the distance of this point from the lens is the principal focal dis- tance. This distance depends upon the curvature of the lens and its refractive power, as measured by the refractive index of the material of which it is composed. Parallel rays are given theo- retically by a source of light at an infinite distance in front of the lens, but practically objects not nearer than about twenty feet give rays so little divergent that they may be considered as par- Fig. 123. — Diagrams to illustrate the refraction of light by a convex lens : a., Refrac- tion of parallel rays ; b., refraction of divergent rays ; c, refraction of divergent rays from a luminous point nearer than the principal focal distance. allel. On the other hand, if a luminous object is placed at F the rays from it that strike upon the lens will emerge from the other surface as parallel rays of light. If a luminous point (/, Fig. 123) is placed in front of such a lens at a distance greater than the principal focal distance, but not so far as to give practically parallel rays, the cone of diverging rays from it that impinges upon the surface of the lens will be brought to a focus (/') further away than the principal focus. Conversely the rays from a luminous point at /' will be brought to a focus at /. These points, / and /', are therefore spoken of as conjugate foci. All luminous 300 THE SPECIAL SENSES. points within the limits specified will have their corresponding conjugate foci, at winch their images will be formed by the lens. Lastly, if a luminous point is placed at v, Fig. 123, nearer to the lens than the principal focal distance, the cone of strongly di- vergent rays that falls upon the lens, although retracted, is still divergent after leaving the lens on the other side and consequently is not focused and forms no real image of the point. For every lens there is a point known as the optical center, and for biconvex lenses this point lies within the lens, o. The line joining this center and the principal focus is the principal axis of the lens {p-F, Fig. 123). All other straight lines passing through the optical center are known as secondary axes. Rays of light that are coincident with any of these secondary axes suffer no angular deviation in passing through the lens; they emerge parallel to their line of entrance and practically unchanged in direction. Moreover, any luminous point not on the I ig. I _' I -Diagram t.i illu frate the formation of an image by a biconvex lens: o, For- mation of the image of a point ; /;, formation of the images of a series of points. principal axis will have its image (conjugate focus) formed some- where upon the secondary axis drawn from this point through the optical center. The exact position of Hie image of such a point can be determined by the following construction (Fig. 121): Let. I represenl the luminous poinl in question. It will throw a cone of rays upon the lens, the limiting rays of which may be represented by A-l) and A-c. One of these rays, .1-/', will be parallel to the prin- cipal axi . and will therefore pass through the principal focus, /''. \i the di tance 18 determined and is indicated properly in the construction, the line .1-// may be drawn, as indicated, so ;is to through F after leaving the lens. The point- at, which the DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 301 prolongation of this line cuts the secondary axis, A-o, marks the conjugate focus of A and gives the position at which all of the rays will be focused to form the image, a. In calculating the position of the image of any object in front of the lens the same method may be followed, the construction being drawn to de- termine the images for two or more limiting points, as shown in Fig. 124. Let A-B be an arrow in front of the lens. The image of A will be formed at a on the secondary axis A-o, and the image of B at b along the secondary axis B-o. The images of the intervening points will, of course, lie between a and b; so that the image of the entire object will be that of an inverted arrow. This image may be caught on a screen at the distance indicated by the construction if the latter is drawn to scale. The principal focus of a convex lens may be determined experimentally or it may be calculated from the formula — + ~ = ~, in which / represents the principal focal dis- tance and p and p1, the conjugate foci for an object farther away than the principal focal distance. That is, if the distance of the object from the lens, p, is known, and the distance of its image, p1, is determined experimentally, the principal focal distance of the lens, /, may be determined by the formula, or if any two of the fac- tors, p, p1, and /, are known the third may be reckoned from the formula. Formation of an Image by the Eye. — As stated above, the re- fractive surfaces of the eye act essentially like a convex lens. As a matter of fact, these refractive surfaces are more complex than in the case of the biconvex lens. In the latter the rays of light suffer refraction at two points only. Where they enter the lens they pass from a rarer to a denser medium and where they leave the lens they pass from a denser to a rarer medium. At these two points, therefore, they are refracted. In the eye there is a larger series of refractive surfaces. The light is refracted at the anterior surface of the cornea, where it passes from the air into the denser medium of the cornea ; at the anterior surface of the lens, where it again enters a denser medium ; and at the posterior surface of the lens, where it enters the less dense vitreous humor. The relative refractive powers of these different media have been determined and are expressed in terms of their refractive indices, that of air being taken as unity.* * The term index of refraction expresses the constant ratio between the angles of incidence and of refraction, or specifically between the sine of the sine i angle of incidence and the sine of the angle of refraction: = index of sine r refraction. 302 THE SPECIAL SENSES. Index of refraction for air = 1 Index of refraction for cornea and aqueous hu- mor = 1.3365 Index of refraction for crystalline lens = 1.4371 Index of refraction for vitreous humor = 1.3365 The three points at which the light is refracted are indicated in the accompanying schema (Fig. 125). The refractive surfaces of the eye may be considered as being composed of a concavo-convex lens, the cornea and aqueous humor, and a biconvex lens, the crystalline lens. In a system of this kind, composed of several refractive media, it has been shown that to construct geometrically the path of the rays it is necessary to know six points ; these are the six car- dinal points or optical con- stants of Gauss, — namely, the anterior and the poste- rior focal distance, the two nodal points, and the two principal points. So far Fig. 125.— Diagram to illustrate the surfaces as the 6Ve is Concerned, it in the eye at which the rays of light are chiefly , , " , , ,-, refracted. has been shown that the path of the rays of light may be represented with sufficient accuracy by employing what is known as the reduced schematic eye of Listing, in which the refraction is supposed to take place at a single convex surface separating two media, the air on one side and the media of the eye on the other, the latter having a refractive index of 1.33 (see Fig. 126). In this reduced eye the position of the ideal refracting surface c' lies in the aqueous humor, a1 a distance of 2.1 mms. from the anterior surface of the cornea, and the position of the nodal poinl or optical center — that is, the center of curvature of the ideal _ ' refracting surface lies in the crystalline lens at n, a ^? distance of 7.:; nuns, from ,:'"- anterior surface of the Fig 126.-Diagram \ QOrnea The principal fo- or schematic eye with a single refracting surface 1 ' eparating two media ol (Efferent densities: <■', Cal distance for this refract- the ideal refrac Burface situated 2. 1 rums. .. .. behind the anterior Burface "I real cornea; ", Urtace lies a1 a UlS- the nodal point, or center of curvature of the ,. ..,, - i • i surface <•', and 15.6 mms. in front of retina. tance ol &\j.l Dims., wniCil The eyeball i supposed to be filled with a uni- n.nl,]j u_ ooiiivolonl (n form substance having a refractive index of 1.33, wr>«i|fl De equivaieni lo equal to that of the vitreou humor. 22.8 mms. (20.7 , 2.1) From the actual surface of the cornea and 15.5 nuns. (22.8— 7.:;) from the nodal point. In the eye a1 resl this principal focal DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 303 distance coincides with the retina, since the refracting surfaces in the normal resting eye are so formed that parallel rays (rays from distant objects) are brought to a focus on the retina. To show the formation of the image of an externa) object on the retina it suffices, therefore, to use a construction such as is represented in Fig. 127. Secondary axes are drawn from the limiting points of the object — A and B — through the nodal point. Where these axes cut the retina the retinal image of the object will be formed. That is, all the rays of light proceeding from A that penetrate the eye will be focused at a, and all proceeding from B at b. The image on the retina will therefore be inverted and will be smaller than the object. The angle formed at the nodal point by the lines A-n and B-n is known as the visual angle; it varies inversely with the dis- tance of the object from the eye. The Inversion of the Image on the Retina. — Although the images of external objects on the retina are inverted, we see them erect. This fact is easily understood when we remember that our actual visual sensations take place in the brain and that the pro- jection of these sensations to the exterior is a secondary act that has been learned from experience. Experience has taught us to project the visual sensation arising from the stimulation of any given point on the retina to that part of the external world from which the stimulus arises — that is, to the luminous point giving origin to the light rays. According to the physi- cal principles described above, the image of such 1 nnint must hp formed FiS- 127- — Diagram to illustrate the construc- a point niUbt Oe lOimeU t.Qn ne^essary to determine the location and size of on the retina where the the retinal image, secondary axis from that point through the nodal point touches the retina. In pro j ecting this retinal stimulus outward to its source, therefore, we have learned to project it back, as it were, along the line of its secondary axis. In Fig. 127 the retinal stimulus at a is projected outward along the line a-n- A, and to such a distance as, from other sources, we estimate the object A to be. This law of projection is fixed by experience, but it implies, as will be noted, that we are conscious of the differences in sensation aroused by stimulation of different parts of the retina. Considering the retina as a sensorv surface, —like the skin, for instance,— each point, speaking in general terms, may be assumed to be connected with a definite portion of the cortex, and the sensation aroused by the stimulation of these dif- ferent points must differ to some extent in consciousness, each has 304 THE SPECIAL SENSES. its local sign. The sensations arising from each of these points we have learned to project outward into the external world along the line from it to the nodal point of the eye, because under the normal conditions of life this point is stimulated only by external objects situated on this line. This law of projection is so firmly fixed that if a given point in the retina is stimulated in some unusual way we still project the resulting sensation outward according to the law, and thus make a false projection and interpretation. For instance, if the little finger is inserted into the inner and lower angle of the eye and is pressed upon the eyeball the edge of the retina is stimulated mechanically. One experiences, in consequence, a visual sensation, known as a phosphene, consisting of a dark-blue spot surrounded hy a light halo. This sensation, however, is projected out toward the upper and outer angle of the eye, accord- ing to the law of projection, since normally this part of the retina is only stimulated by light coming from such a direction. A similar error in projection is obtained by holding objects so close to the eye that a physical inverted image cannot be formed, but only an erect shadow image. This experiment may be performed as follows: Hold the head of a pin close to the eye, and, in order that a sharp shadow may be thrown, allow the light to fall on this pin through a pinhole in a card held somewhat farther from the eye. By this means an erect shadow of the pin, lying in the circle of light from the hole, will be thrown on the eye. This shadow image will be projected outward according to the usual law, and consequently will appear inverted. The Size of the Retinal Image. — The size of the image of an object on the retina may be reckoned easily, provided the size of the object and its distance from the eye is known. As will be seen from the construction given in Fig. 1-7, the triangles A-n-B and i-bare symmetrical; consequently we have the ratio: A-B : a-b : : A-n : a -n or A-B A-n — r— = ; lluil is a I. a-n Size of object I>i lance of object from nodal point. Size of lira : Distance of image from nodal point. As was i tated above, the distance of the image From the nodal point thai is, the distance of the retina from the nodal point- inn.' be placed al L5.5or L5mms. Consequently, three of the factors in the above equation being known, it is easily solved for the un- known factor namely, the size of the image on the retina. To take a concrete example; i uppo e h is desired to know the size on tin- retina of the image made by an object 120 feet high at- a distance of one mile (5280 feet), [f we designate the size of the image as z DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 305 and substitute the known values for the other terms of the equation, we have — = 4r, ori = 0.341 mm., which is about the cliam- eter of the fovea centralis. The retinal image of the object in this case would be, in round numbers, about to-q Vut of the actual height of the object. Accommodation of the Eye for Objects at Different Dis- tances.— The normal or, as it is sometimes named, the emmetropic eye, is arranged to focus parallel rays more or less accurately upon the retina. That is, the refractive media have such curvatures and densities that parallel, or substantially parallel rays are brought to a focus upon the retinal surface. When objects are brought closer to the eye, however, the rays proceeding from them become more and more divergent. If the eye remains unchanged the refracted rays cut the retina before coming to a focus — so that each luminous point in the object, instead of forming a point upon the Fig. 128. — Diagram explaining the change in the position of the image reflected from the anterior surface of the crystalline lens. — (.Williams, after Donders.) retina, forms a circle, known as a diffusion circle. As this is true for each point of the object, the retinal image as a whole is blurred. We know, however, that up to a certain point at least this blurring does not occur when the object is brought closer to the eyes. The eye, in fact, accommodates itself to the nearer object so as to obtain a clear focus. In a photographic camera this accommodation or focusing is effected by moving the ground glass plate farther awav as the object is brought closer to the 20 306 THE SPECIAL SENSES. lens. In the eve the same result is obtained by increasing the curva- ture and therefore the refractive power of the lens. That a change in the lens is the essential factor in accommodation for near objects is demonstrated by a simple and conclusive experiment devised by Helmholtz with the aid of what are known as the images of Pur- kinje. The principle of this experiment is represented by the dia- gram given in Fig. 128. The eye to be observed is relaxed; that is, gazes into the distance. A lighted candle is held to one side as represented, and the observer places his eye so as to catch the light of the candle when reflected from the observed eye. With a little practice and under the right conditions of illumina- tion the observer will be able to see three images of the candle re- A B Fiir. 129. — Reflected images of a candle flame as seen in the pupil of an eye at rest and accommodated for near objects. — (Williams.) fleeted from the observed eye as from a mirror: one, the brightest, is reflected from the convex surface of the cornea (a, Fig. 129, A); one much dimmer and of larger size is reflected from the convex surface of the lens {b, Fig. 129, .1). This image; is larger and hunter because the reflecting surface is less curved. The third image (c, Fig. 129, .1) is inverted and is smaller and brighter than the second. This image is reflected from the posterior surface of the lens, which acts, in this instance, like a concave mirror. If now the observed eye gazes at a near object, it will he noted (Fig. 120. H) t hat the first image does not change at all, the third image also remains practically the same, but the middle image (b) becomes smaller and approaches nearer to the fii'-t (a). This result can only menu th;it in the net of accom- modation the anterior surface of the lens becomes more convex. In this w:iv its refractive power is increased mid the more diver gent rays from the near object are focused on the retina. Ilelm- holtz has shown that the curval lire of the posterior surface of the leu- i- also Increased slightly; but the change is so slight that the increased refractive power is referred chiefly to the change in the anterior Burface. The means by which the change is effected DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 307 was first explained satisfactorily by Helmholtz.* He attributed it to the contraction of the ciliary muscle. This small muscle, composed of plain muscle fibers, is found within the eyeball, lying between the choroid and the sclerotic coat at the point at which the sclerotic passes into the cornea and the choroid falls into the ciliary processes. Some of its fibers take a more or less circular direction around the eyeball, resembling thus a sphincter muscle, while others take a radial direction in the planes of the meridians of the eye and have their insertion in the choroid coat (Fig. 130). When this muscle contracts the radial fibers especially will pull forward the choroid coat. The effect of this change in the choroid is to loosen the pull of the suspensory ligament (zonula Zinnii) on the lens and this organ then bulges forward by its own elasticity. The theory assumes that in a condition of rest the suspensory ligament, which runs from the ciliary processes to the capsule of the lens, exerts a Ciliary Border process, of iris. Ciliary muscle. l>ens Pigment y epithelium, Ora serrata. Fig. 130. — Meridional section of eyeball after removal of sclerotic coat, cornea, and iris, to show the position of the ciliary muscle. — (Schultze.) tension upon the lens which keeps it flattened, particularly along its anterior surface, since the ligament is attached more to this side. When this tension is relieved indirectly by the contraction of the ciliary muscle the elasticity of the lens, or rather of the capsule of the lens, causes it to assume a more spherical shape along its anterior surface, and the amount of this change is proportional to the extent of contraction of the muscle. Other theories have been proposed to explain the way in which the contraction of the ciliary * Helmholtz, "Handbuch der phvsiologischen Optik," second edition, 1896. 30S THE SPECIAL SENSES. muscle effects a change in the curvature of the lens,* but none is so simple and, on the whole, so satisfactory as the one suggested bv Helmholtz. It is interesting to note that in fishes accommodation is effected in a different way, namely, by movements of the lens forward and backward. In these animals the eye when at rest is accommodated for near vision, and in sec objects at a distance the refractive power of the eye is diminished by the contraction of a special muscle, retractor lentis, which pulls the lens toward the retina. t Limit of the Power of Accommodation — Near Point of Distinct Vision. — When an object is brought closer and closer to the eye a point will be reached at which it is impossible by the strongest contraction of the ciliary muscle to obtain a clear image of the object. The rays from it are so divergent that the refractive surfaces are unable to bring them to a focus on the retina. Each luminous point makes a diffusion circle on the retina, and the whole image is indistinct. The distance at which the eye is just able to accommodate and within which distinct vision is impos- sible is called the near point. Observation shows that this near point varies steadily with age and becomes rapidly greater in dis- tance between the fortieth and the fiftieth year. In the case of the normal eye the recession of the near point varies so regularly with age that its determination may be used to estimate the age of the individual. Figures of this kind are given : Age. Near Point. 10 7 cm. or 2.76 in. 20 " 10 " " 3.94 " 30 14 " " 5.61 " Ki 22 " " 8.66 " 50 40 " " 15.75 " 60. '.'.'. 100 " " 39.37 " This gradual lengthening of the near point is explained usually by the supposition that the lens loses its elasticity, so that con- traction of the ciliary muscle has less and less effect in causing an increase in its curvature. The process starts very early in life, and is one of the many facts which show that senescence begins practically with birth. The change in near point in early life is so Blighl as bO escape notice, but after it reaches a distance of about 25 cm. (aboul 10 inches) the fact obtrudes itself upon us in the use of our eves for near objects, reading, for example. The condition i then designated as old-sightedness or presbyopia. Most normal eves become so distinctly presbyopic between the fortieth and the fiftieth year ae to require the use of glasses in reading. If no other defed exist in the eye, this deficiency of the lens is readily over- * Bee Tscherning, "Optique phyeiologique," Paris, L898; and Schoen, " Archiv f. die gei ammte Physiologic." 59, 427, ix!).r>. ee \\i V Motor jiupillae. Superior Ceruieal/ Ganglion/ ~? / Q/ju/ial Cord A / 3° 'Cranial W / nerve. CuwryGanjlvoru Short Ciliary heroes Ceruieal SjmpaVietic Fig. 135. — Schema showing the path of the preganglionic and postganglionic fibers to the ciliary mu.scle and to the sphincter and dilator muscles of the iris. — (Modified from Schultz.) The course of the long ciliary nerves is represented very diagrammatically. sympathetic, for instance — the pupil is seen to contract. This constant activity may be referred directly to the activity of the spinal neurons whose cells lie in the spinal cord in the lower cervical and upper thoracic region. The cells in question constitute what is sometimes called the lower ciliospinal center of Budge. The Accommodation Reflex and the Light Reflex of the Sphincter Muscle.- When the eye is accommodated for a near object by the contraction of the ciliary muscle there is always a simultaneous contraction of the sphincter pupillse whereby the pupil is narrowed. The net is one of obvious value in vision, since by diaphragming down the lens the focus is improved and more exacl vision, such ae is needed in close work, is obtained. The act uallv spoken of as the accommodation reflex, hut in reality it DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 319 is rather what is known as an associated movement. The voluntary effort inaugurated in the brain affects the cranial centers for both muscles, and under normal conditions they always act together, — a fact which implies a close connection of their centers. An example of a similar associated action is seen in the effect of the respiratory movements on the rate of heart beat, the inspirator}' discharge from the respiratory center being accompanied by an associated effect upon the cardio-inhibitory center whereby the heart rate is quickened. In the particular case that we are dealing with three muscular acts, in fact, are usually associated, for every act of accommodation under normal circumstances is accompanied not only by a constriction of the pupil, but also by a convergence of the eyeballs, due to a contraction of the internal rectus muscle in each eye. The light reflex is observed when light is thrown into the eye. As is well known, the pupil dilates in darkness or dim lights and contracts to a pin-point upon strong illumination of the retina. The value of this reflex is also obvious. In the dim light the total illumination and therefore the visual power of the retina is aided by an enlarged pupil, but in strong lights the illumination may be diminished with advantage by diaphragming, since the optical image on the retina is thereby improved on account of the diminu- tion in spherical aberration. The reflex arc involved in this act is known in part. The afferent path is along the optic nerve; the efferent path back to the sphincter is through the third nerve and ciliary ganglion ; injury to either of these paths diminishes or destroys the reflex. The reflex is also lost in some cases in which neither of these paths seems to be involved. In tabes dorsalis (locomotor ataxia) and general paresis, for instance, the pupil of the eye is constricted and does not give the light reflex, but still shows the accommodation reflex. Such a condition is known as the Argyll Robertson pupil. Some question exists, there- fore, as to the nature of the connections in the brain between the afferent impulses and the motor center in the nucleus of the third nerve. According to some authors (Gudden, v. Bechterew), the afferent light reflex fibers are a set of fibers distinct from the visual fibers proper. They arise in the retina and pass backward in the optic nerve, but leave the optic tracts at the chiasma to enter the walls of the third ventricle and thus reach the nucleus of the third nerve. This view, however, finds no support in the histological structure of the retina. Under normal conditions the light reflex is bilateral, — that is, light thrown upon one retina only will cause constriction of the pupil in both eyes. In those of the lower ani- mals whose optic nerves cross completely in the chiasma the light reflex, on the contrary, is unilateral, affecting only the eye that 320 THE SPECIAL SENSES. is stimulated.* We may conclude, therefore, that the bilaterality of the reflex in the higher animals is dependent upon the partial decussation of the optic fibers in the chiasma, a sensor}' stimulus upon one retina giving rise to impulses which are conveyed to the two sides of the brain. It is possible, however, that in addition commissural connections may exist between the central connections, — the motor centers in the midbrain. It is usually stated that the effect of the light upon the sphincter muscle is greatest when the retina is stimulated at or near the fovea and that it varies directly with the intensity of the light and the area illuminated.t The Action of Drugs upon the Iris. — The condition of con- striction of the pupil is frequently designated as miosis (mi-o'-sis) and the condition of dilatation as mydriasis (myd-ri'-as-is). Many drugs are known which, when applied directly to the absorptive surfaces of the eye or when injected into the circulation, affect the muscles of the iris and therefore vary the size of the pupil. Those drugs that cause miosis are spoken of as miotics, and those that produce mydriasis as mydriatics. Atropin, the active prin- ciple of belladonna, homatropin, and cocain are well-known myd- riatics, while physostigmin (eserin) and muscarin or pilocarpin are examples of the miotics. There has been much question as to the precise action of these drugs. For an adequate discussion of this question the student is referred to works on pharmacology; but it may be said that the evidence from the physiological sidej indi- cates that atropin causes mydriasis by paralyzing the endings of the constrictor nerve fibers in the sphincter muscle, while phy- sostigmin and muscarin cause miosis by -stimulation of the endings of these same fibers. In the case of cocain it is probable that the drug first stimulates mainly the endings of the dilator fibers in the dilator muscles, and in stronger doses causes additional mydriasis by paralyzing the constrictor fibers. The stronger mydriatics paralyze not only the sphincter pupillaj, but also the similarly innervated ciliary muscle, thus destroying the power of accom- modation. When atropin is applied to the eye the individual is unable to use his eyes for near work — reading, for example — until the effect of the drug has worn off. In ophthalmological literature this condition of paralysis of the ciliary muscle is spoken of as cycloplegia, and most of the mydriatic drugs are also cycloplcgics. On the contrary, the stronger miotics stimulate the ciliary muscle, and therefore during their period of action throw the eye into a condition of forced accommodation. • Steinach, "Archiv f. d. gesammte Physiologic," 47, 313, 1800. f Sec Abelsdorfi and Feilchenfell, "Zeitschnit f. Psychologic unci Phys- iologic dee Sinnesorgane," 34, 111, L904. JSchultz, "Archiv f. Physiologic," 1898, 47. DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 321 In the above description of the innervation of the iris and the causes of mydriasis and miosis the simplest explanations offered have been adopted. It should be added, however, that some facts are known which indicate that the conditions are more complex, particularly in regard to the dilator mechan- ism. P"or example, adrenalin applied to the eye causes dilatation of the pupil, but with varying degrees of rapidity for different eyes, the least rapidly for the eyes of those animals which are most sensitive to light (Schultz). When the superior cervical ganglion is removed, on the contrary, the dilating action of the adrenalin is much more rapid. (Meltzer and Auer.) The Balanced Action of the Sphincter and Dilator Muscles of the Iris. — It would seem that under normal conditions both the sphincter and the dilator muscle are kept more or less in tonic activity by impulses received through their respective motor fibers. They thus balance each other, to speak figuratively, and a mechan- ism of this kind in which two opposing actions are in play is in a condition to respond promptly and smoothly to an excess of stimu- lation from either side. The two muscles, in fact, act as antago- nists in the same manner as the flexor and extensor muscles around a joint. At the same time this relation adds some difficulties to the explanation of specific reactions, since it is evident that a dila- tation of the pupil may be caused either by a contraction of the dilator muscle or a loss of tone (inhibition) in the sphincter, while in constriction of the pupil the effect may result either from a con- traction of the sphincter or an inhibition of the dilator; or, last, the contraction of one muscle may always be accompanied by an inhibition of its antagonist, as is supposed to be the case with the flexor and extensor muscles of the limbs * (law of reciprocal in- nervation). Anderson has given some evidence to show that the dilatation of the pupil in cats is due to a double action of this sort, the pupillodilator muscle contracting first and subsequently the tone of the constrictors suffering an inhibition. Alterations in the size of the pupil take place not only under the conditions described above — namely, the light and the accommodation reflex and the action of drugs — but also under many other cir- cumstances, normal and pathological. In sleep, for instance, the eyes roll upward and inward and the pupils are constricted. It would seem probable that the miosis in this case is due to a cessation in tonic activity on the part of the dilator muscle rather than to an active contraction of the sphincter muscle, the state of sleep being characterized by a diminution in activity in the central nervous system. Emotional states also affect the size of the pupil and thus aid in giving the facial expressions character- istic of these conditions. Writers speak of the eyes dilating with terror or darkening with emotions of deep pleasure. This pupil- * " Journal of Physiology," 30, 15, 1903. 21 322 THE SPECIAL SENSES. lary accompaniment of the emotional states may occur even when it is a matter of memory rather than immediate experience. The explanation of this mydriasis can hardly be obtained by experi- ment, but reasoning from analogy we know that strong emotional states are usually accompanied by more or less distinct inhibitory effects on motor centers, and perhaps in tins case the reaction is most satisfactorily explained by attributing it to an inhibition of the constrictor center in the midbrain. Intraocular Pressure.— The liquids in the interior of the eye are normally under a pressure, the average value of which may be estimated at 25 mms. of mercury. In consequence of this internal pressure the eyeball is tense and its external surface, including the cornea, shows a regular curvature. It is obvious that folds or creases in the cornea would entirely destroy its use- fulness, so far as the formation of an image is concerned. The amount of the intraocular pressure may be measured by thrusting a tubular needle, properly connected with a manometer, into the anterior chamber of the eye. The liquid in the interior of the eyeball may be considered as tissue lymph, and like the lymph- elsewhere it is derived from the blood-plasma. Investigation has shown that the lymph is formed in the ciliary processes, but in this as in other cases there is a difference of opinion as to whether the production is due to so-called secretory or to mechanical causes, such as filtration. The facts that are known seem to be explicable from the mechanical point of view.* We may suppose that the liquid niters into the eye through the vessels in the ciliary processes, and, on the other hand, drains off at the angle of the anterior chamber through the canal of Schlemm. The intraocular pressure rises until, under its influence, the out- flow just balances the inflow. It is evident from this point of view that intraocular pressure' will be increased by any change that will augment the production of the liquid at the ciliary processes, such as a rise of blood-pressure, or by any interference with the outflow, such as might arise from a blocking of the canal of Schlemm. Certain pathological conditions (glaucoma) are characterized by an abnormally high intraocular tension, the difference from the normal being such that it is easily recognized by pressure with the fingers. Methods of Determining the Refraction of the Eye. The condition of the eye a regards its refraction may be determined by the use of suitable charts and a Beries of spherical and cylindrical lenses. The results n a method depend largely upon the statements of the patient, that bey are Largely subjective. A uumber of instruments have been I, however, by means of which the refraction of the eye may be studied For di cu ion and Literature, see Henderson and Starling, 'Proceed- ing Royal Society," L906, B. Lxxvii. DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 323 in a purely objective way, so far as the patient is concerned. The most important of these instruments are the ophthalmoscope, the retinoscope or skiascope, and the ophthalmometer. A brief description is given of each of these instruments, but for the numerous practical details necessary to their successful use reference must be made to special manuals. The Ophthalmoscope. — The light that falls into the eye is partly ab- sorbed by the black pigment of the choroid coat and is partly reflected back to the exterior. This latter portion is reflected back in the direction in which it entered. Merely holding a light near the eye does not, therefore, enable us to see the interior more clearly, since in order to catch the returning rays in our own eye it would be necessary to interpose the head between the source of light and the observed eye. If, however, we could ar- range the light to enter the observed eye as though it proceeded from our own eye, then the returning rays would be per- ceived, and with sufficient illumination the bottom or fundus of the observed eye might be seen. Arguing in this way, Helmholtz constructed his first form of the ophthalmoscope in 1851. The value of the ophthalmoscope is twofold: It en- ables the observer to examine the interior of the eye and thus recognize diseased conditions of the retina; it is also useful in detecting abnormalities in the refrac- tive surfaces of the eye. The principle of the instrument is well represented in the original form devised by Helmholtz, as shown schematically in Fig. 137, A. I represents the observed eye and II the eye of the observer. Between the two eyes is placed a piece of glass inclined at an angle. Light from the candle falling upon this glass is in part reflected from the surface to enter eye /, and these rays on emerging from the eye along the same line pass through the glass in part and enter eye //. In place of the plane un- silvered glass it is now customary to use a concave mirror with a small hole through the center, the observer's eye being placed directly behind this hole. Such an in- strument is shown in Fig. 136, The in- strument is used in two ways, known as the direct and the indirect method. In the direct method the mirror is held very close to the observed eye and the paths of the rays of light into and out of the eye are represented schematically in Fig. 137, B. The light from a lamp caught upon the mirror is thrown into the eye, the rays coming to a focus and then spreading out so as to give a diffuse illumination of the fundus. This latter surface may now be considered as a luminous object sending out rays of light. Taking any three objects on the retina, A, B, C, it is apparent that if eye I is an emmetropic eye these points are at the principal focal dis- tance and the rays sent from each after emerging from the eye are in parallel bundles. These rays penetrate the hole in the mirror and fall into the ob- server s eye as though they came from distant objects. If the observer's eye is also emmetropic, or is made so by suitable glasses, these bundles of rays will be focused on his retina without an act of accommodation. He must, in fact, in looking through the mirror, gaze, not at the eye before him, but, re- Fig. 136. — Loring's ophthalmoscope. 324 THE SPECIAL SENSES. taxing his accommodation, gaze through the eye. as it were, into the distance. ]n this way he will see the portion of the retina illuminated, the image of the objects seen being inverted on liis own retina and therefore projected or seen erect. If the observed eye is myopic its retina is farther back than the prin- cipal focus of its refracting surfaces; consequently the rays sent out from the illuminated retina emerge in converging bundles and cannot be focused on the retina of the observer's eye. By inserting a concave lens of proper power between his eye and the mirror the observer can render the rays parallel and thus bring out the image. From the power of the lens used the degree of my- opia may be estimated. Just the reverse happens if the observed eye is hypermetropic. In such an eye the retina is nearer than the principal focal .1 137. Diagrams to represent the principle of the ophthalmoscope: A, The orig- inal form of ophthalmoscope, consisting of a piece of glass, .i/, inclined at a suitable angle. j from the source of light are reflected into the observed e\r, /, and i hence return along the same lines passing through .1/ to reach the observer"* eye, //. li, the direct method with the ophthalmoscopic mirror. The rays of light illuminate the fundus of the observed eye, /, and thence pass out in parallel rays, if the eye is emmetropic, to reach the e //. C, the indirect method with ophthalmoscopic mirror ami intercalated i be rays of light-red lines are brought to a locus within the anterior chamber of the I thence diverge to give a general illumination of the interior of the eyeball. The returning rays of light are indicated foi a ingle point, b. At a', f, i '. a real inverted image of a portion of the retina it formed in the air, which in turn is focused on the retina of the observer1 eye. distance of the refractive Burface; consequently the lij,rht emitted from the retina emerges in bundles of diverging rays which cannot be broughl to a focus on the retina of the observer unless he exerts his own power of accom- modation or interposes a convex lens between his eye and the mirror. The indirect method of using the ophthalmoscope is represented in Fig. The mirror is held al some distance, a1 arm's length, from bhe ob- served eye, /. while just before this eye a biconvex lens of shoii focus is placed, fit shown in the diagram by the red lines, the reflected light from the mirror comes to a focus and then diverging falls upon the biconvex lens. Tbii leni brings tin- rays to a focus at or near the eye, whence they again diverge and light up the retina with a diffuse illumination. The lighl from DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 325 this retina is in turn sent back toward the mirror, its path being indicated for the point b by the black lines. If the eye is emmetropic the rays from this point emerge parallel, and falling upon the biconvex lens are brought to a focus at b'. Similarly the rays from a will be brought to a focus at a' and from c at c' . Consequently there will be formed in the air an inverted image, and it is at this image that the eye of the observer gazes through the hole in the mirror. This image forms its image on the retina of the observer's eye, as represented in the diagram at a", b" ', c" , and is projected outward or seen inverted as regards the original position of the points in the retina of eye /. The indirect method is the one usually employed in ophthalmoscopic exam- inations of the retina. It gives a larger field than the direct method, although the objects seen are of smaller size. The Retinoscope or Skiascope. — When one reflects a spot of light upon a wall, any movement of the reflecting (plane) mirror is followed by a movement of the reflected spot in the same direction. So if the fundus of the eye is illuminated by a plane mirror provided with a peep-hole, the observer looking through this hole may see a spot of light reflected from the retina and can determine whether the spot moves in the same direction as the mirror or against it. If the eye under observation is normal (emmetropic), then the rays of light starting from the retina will emerge in parallel bundles, since the retina lies at the principal focal distance, and as the mirror is tilted from side to side the illuminated spot moves in the same direction. By placing a convex lens of suitable focus in front of the observed eye we can cause the emerging parallel rays to come to a focus and cross before reaching the ob- server's eye. In such a case the movements of the spot of light upon the retina will be against those of the mirror. For example, let us suppose that the observing eye is placed just 1 meter away from the eye observed, then if we put in front of the latter a convex lens of 1.25 D. the emerging rays will be focused at a point 25 ctm. in front of the observer's eye and the movements of the spot of light will be against the mirror. A lens of less than 1 D. placed in front of the observed eye would not bring the rays to a focus in front of the observer's retina, consequently the movements of the spot would be with the mirror. Assuming that we are dealing with an emmetropic eye, it can be shown that at the distance mentioned (1 meter) any lens of less than 1 D. placed in front of the eye leaves the movements with the mirror, while any lens of more than 1 D. gives movements against the mirror. Consequently a lens of just 1 D. would mark the exact "point of rever- sal. " With a lens of tins power the focus would fall theoretically just on the observer's retina. In such a case any movement of the mirror would be followed by the appearance or disappearance of the spot, but no direction of movement would be perceived. The movements of the spot of light formed upon the retina by the retinoscopic mirror may be used to determine all the various abnormalities of refraction of the eye according to the following general schema : The observer sits at a fixed distance, say 1 meter, from the patient, and determines whether the reflected spot from the illuminated fundus moves with or against the mirror. If the movement is with the mirror, then the eye under observation is either normal or hyperopic (or if myopic the myopia is less than 1 D.). By placing convex lenses in front of the eye the observer seeks for the point of reversal. If this point is given by a lens of + 1 D., then the eye under examination is emmetropic ; if»a stronger lens is required the eye is hyperopic, that is, the emerging rays are divergent and require a stronger lens to bring them to a focus before reaching the observer's eye. In the latter case the amount of hyperopia is obtained by ascertaining the strength in diopters of the lens required to just reverse the movement and subtracting 1 D. from it, since the latter amount is required, at a distance of 1 meter,' to get reversal with the normal eye. If the reversal is given by a convex lens of less than 1 D., then the eye is myopic to an extent less than 1 D. When the movements of the spot of light are against the mirror from the beginning, then the observer is dealing with a myopic eye (the myopia being greater than 1 D.). To reverse the movement it is now necessary to place concave lenses in front of the observed eye until the point of reversal is obtained, that is, until the focus of the emerging rays falls behind the 326 THE SPECIAL SENSES. retina of the observer. The concave lens necessary to give this result, plus 1 D. for distance, gives the extent of the myopia in diopters. With astigmatic eyes tae point of reversal may be determined for the different meridians of the eye, tne movements of the mirror being in the same meridian. By the character of tiie reflected spot and the points of reversal it is possible with tae retinoscope to determine the principal meridians, and the difference in refraction between them, that is, the degree and the axis of the astigmatism. The Ophthalmometer. — The ophthalmometer is an instrument for measuring tae curvature of the refracting surfaces of the eye. As actually applied in practise it is arranged especially for measuring the curvatures of the cornea along its different meridians. The point for which the instru- ment is designed is to obtain the size of the image reflected from the convex surface of the cornea. Any luminous object placed in front of the eye will give a reflected image from the cornea as from the surface of a convex mirror. If the size of the object and its distance from the cornea are known and the size of the corneal image is determined, then the radius of curvature of the cornea is given by the equation r — _ Pl, in which p represents the distance of o Fig. V.ifi. — Schema to indicate the general principle of ll phthahuoineter : T, Telescope to observe the reflected images from the cornea; .1 and />', the targets or mires in the shield at a known distance apart whose images are reflected from the Cornea; a and b, the rellected images of A and B on the cornea. The distance a b has to be determined. the object from the cornea, i, the rfze of the corneal image, and o, the size of the object. For example, let A and B in Fig. 138 be two luminous areas arranged on the arc of a circle. If placed in front of the cornea C each will give a reflected image a and l>, which may be observed by means of the telescope T. The distance between A and />' represents the size of the object and the distance between n and l> the size of the image. This latter factor i.» determined by means of the telescope. A scale, for in- stance, mighl I"- placed in the eye-piece of the telescope and the distance o-4 be determined in terms of its graduation. This valuation might then be K>nverted into milHmeters by substituting a scale for the cornea and measuring off upon ii the observed distance in the eye-piece scale. If the .arc carrying .1/; i arranged so thai it may be rotated it is obvious thai the size of the corneal images may be measured for the differenl meridians and thus enable one i" compare their curvatures, in modern instruments, such as is repre- sented in I h'. L39, the luminou areas, known as targets or mires, are placed in a Spherical shield which may be rotated around the axis of the t elescope. The shield ha a 1:1 < I i 1 1 of curva t ure of 0.35 meters ami its center of rotation is approximately coincidenl with thai of the cornea when the eye is in its proper position. The rellected images of the mires from the surface of the DIOPTRICS OF THE EYE. 327 cornea are each doubled, when viewed through the telescope, by means of a double vision prism of Iceland spar and the displacement produced in this way is a definite amount for the distance chosen. Four images of the mires are thus seen, and when the mires are properly adjusted for a cornea of average curvature the two inner images are in contact with each other. A variation from this average is indicated by an overlapping of the images, Fig. 139.— Ophthalmometer (Hardy). the value of which in diopters or in radii of curvature is read off upon the instrument. The instrument, therefore, when once calibrated enables one to read off at once the radii of curvature for the different meridians and thus determine the axis and degree of astigmatism. It should be added that the instrument gives only the curvatures and degree of astigmatism, if any exists, of the cornea, and is therefore of no immediate service in determining the total astigmatism, that is, the astigmatism of cornea and lens acting together. CHAPTER XVIII. THE PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA— VISUAL STIMULI AND VISUAL SENSATIONS. The Portion of the Retina Stimulated by Light. — The normal stimulus to the sensory cells in the retina is found in the vibrations of the ether, the waves of light. When sunlight is passed through a prism the waves of different lengths are dispersed, and those capable of stimulating the retina form the visible spectrum extending from red to violet. The limits of the spectrum are, on the one hand, the extreme red rays with a wave length of TTmrWff mm- and vibrating at the rate of about 390,000,000,000,000 a second, and, on the other, the extreme violet, having a wave length of about TFtHrlnny mm- aQd a rate of vibration of 770,000,000,000,000 a second. The part of the retina stimulated by these vibrations is supposed to be the layer of rods and cones. To reach these structures the light must pass Fig. 140. — To demonstrate the blind spot. Fix the center of the cross with the right eve, then move the book slowly to or from the fare. At a certain distance the image of the large circle to the right will disappear. At this distance the image of the circle falls on the optii di e. through the oilier layers of the retina. That the rods and cones arc the structures that react to the light stimulation is indicated by their structure and their connections and by such facts as the follow- ing: Under certain conditions, which are described below, the shadows of the retinal vessels and the contained corpuscles may be seen, a fact which indicates that the perceiving structures lie ex- ternally to these Vessels, fn the fovea centralis, in which vision is most perfect , the layers of the retina are thinned out until practically only the rod- and cone remain to be acted upon. That, the optic fibers themselves are no1 acted upon by li^hi waves is proved by the existence of the blind spot. The termination of the optic nerve within the eyeball, the optic disc, lies about L5 degrees to the Qfl $al side of the fovea and has a diameter of about 1.5 inms. From t his point t he nerve-libers spread out over 1 he rest of I he optic cup to form the internal layer of t he pel in a. But the optic disc itself has 328 PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 329 no retinal structure, and light that falls upon it is not perceived. The presence of this blind spot in our visual field is easily demon- strated by the experiment illustrated and described in Fig. 140. In the visual field for each eye, therefore, there is a gap representing the projection of the area of the optic disc to the exterior, the size of the gap increasing with the distance from the eye. We do not notice this deficiency, inasmuch as it exists in our indirect field of vision (see below), in which our perception of form is poorly de- veloped ; so that any disturbance in outline that might result in the retinal image of external objects is unperceived. Morever, the portion of the external world that falls on the blind spot of one eye falls on the retinal field of the other, and is thus perceived in binoc- ular vision. It is to be borne in mind, also, that the projection of the blind spot does not appear in the visual field as a dark area; it is simply an absent area, so that no gap exists in our consciousness of the spatial relations of the visual field; the margins, so to speak, of the hole come into contact so far as our consciousness is concerned. The Action Current Caused by Stimulation of the Retina. — The effect of light waves falling upon the retina is to set up a series of nerve impulses in the optic nerve fibers. It is interesting to find that these impulses aroused in a sensory nerve by a normal stimulus are attended by electrical changes similar to those observed in motor fibers when stimulated normally or artificially. The fact strengthens the view that the electrical change is an invariable ac- companiment of the nerve impulse, if not the nerve impulse itself. If the eye is excised and connected with a galvanometer or capillary electrometer by two non-polarizable electrodes, one placed upon the cut end of the optic nerve and the other on the cornea, the usual demarcation current is obtained due to the injury to the optic nerve. ' If the preparation is kept in the dark and arrangements are made to throw a light through the pupil upon the retina the galvanometer indicates an electrical change or current whenever the light is admitted.* The direction of the current in the eyeball is from the fundus to the cornea, and as regards the pre-existing demarcation current it is in the same direction and forms, therefore, a so-called positive variation. When the electrodes are placed on the longi- tudinal and the cut surface of the optic nerve, then, according to Ktihne, the electrical response to light is a negative variation similar to that described for stimulation of nerves in general (p. 103). Not only is there a "light response" each time that the retina is stimu- lated by light, but there is a similar electrical change, a " dark re- sponse," when the light is suddenly withdrawn. This last- interesting fact would seem to indicate a stimulation process of some kind in the retina due to darkness — that is, withdrawal * Dewar and McKendrick, " Transactions, Royal Society, Edinburgh," 27, 1873; Got«h, "Journal of Physiology," 29, 388, 1903, and 31, 1, 1904. 330 THE SPECIAL SENSES. of the objective stimulus. Einthoven and Jolly* have applied the sensitive string galvanometer to the study of this phenome- non. They find that the electrical response of the illuminated eye. when photographed, presents a curve of much complexity. and they conclude that its complexity is due to the fact that several different processes occur together in the stimulated retina. They offer some evidence to indicate that three different processes depending on the reaction of three different substances may be distinguished. These substances react with different- velocities ami with different changes in electric potential to Mashes of light and "flashes of darkness." What physiological effects may be connected with these three processes cannot yet be stated. The electrical reaction is a very sensitive one, lights so weak as to be near the threshold for the human eye give a distinct electrical change in the frog's retina, and an eye that has been kept in the dark for some time (dark-adapted eye) shows an increased sensitiveness. It is very interesting, also, to find that the frog's retina responds to a range of light vibrations that corresponds with the limits of the visible spectrum as seen by the human eye. If the electrical response is a true indication of functional activity, it would appear that the frog's vision has about the same extent as our own as regards the ether waves of different periods of vibration. The Visual Purple -Rhodopsin. — The change that takes place in the rods and cones whereby the vibratory energy of the ether waves is converted into nerve impulses is unknown. It has been assumed by some observers that the light waves act mechanically, the wave movements setting into vibration portions of the external segments of the rods or cones, and that this mechanical movement forms the direct excitant of the nerve impulses, f The general view, however, is that the process is photochemical, — that is, the impact of the ether waves sets up chemical changes in the rods or cones which in turn give rise to nerve impulses that arc transmitted to the brain. We have an analogy for this action in the known change produced by light upon sensitized photographic films. In the retina itself some basis for such a view is found in the existence of a red pigment which is bleached by light. This interesting dis- COVery was made by Boll, J and the facts were afterward carefully investigated by Kiihne.§ The red pigment, known usually as visual purple or rhodopsin, is t mind only in the external segments * Einthoven and .Jolly, "Quarterly Journal of experimental Physiology," I, 373, L908. | /■ a] < i. ■■ Aidiiv f. mil.. \ 1 1 : 1 1 < . 1 1 1 i < ■ , " :;, 248, is(>7. J Boll, "Archiv t Physiologie," 1877, 4. § Kuhne, "Untersucn. a. of. physiol. Inst. d. Qniv. Heidelberg," vol. i, IS7S. Also "The Photochemistry of the Retina," etc., translated by Foster, London, 1878. PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 331 of the rods; the cones do not contain it. In the fovea, therefore, which has only cones, the pigment is entirely absent. The existence of the visual purple may be demonstrated very easily. A frog is kept for some time in the dark; it is then killed and an eye removed and bisected equatorially. If the vitreous is removed from the pos- terior half the retina may be detached by means of a pair of forceps. When the operation is performed in red or yellow light, as in photo- graphic work, the detached retina on examination by daylight is. found to be a deep-red color; but after a short exposure it fades rapidly, finally becoming colorless. If the frogs before operation were exposed to strong daylight, the retina is found to be colorless. A similar pigment is found in the eyes of man and the other mammalia. It has been shown, moreover, that a photo- graph may be made upon the surface of the retina by means of this purple. If the head of a rabbit or frog that has been kept in the dark for some time is exposed with proper precautions to the light of a window, for instance, the part of the retina on which the image of the window-lights falls will be bleached, while the parts upon which the image of the window-bars falls and the surrounding areas of the retina will retain their red color. A figure of such a retinal photograph or optogram, as it is called, is represented in the accom- panying illustration (Fig. 141). The visual purple has been extracted from the rods by solutions of bile salts, this substance having the power to discharge the pigment from its combination in the rods in the same way as it discharges hemoglobin from its combination in the red corpuscles. The solutions thus obtained are also bleached upon exposure to light. We have in the visual purple, therefore, an unstable substance readily decomposed or altered by the me- chanical effect of the ether waves, and also, it may ' be said, by gross mechanical re- actions, such as com- 0 preSSlOn; and there Fig. 141.— Optogram in eye of rabbit: 1, The nor- Can be little doubt ma^ appearance of the retina in the rabbit's eye: a. The entrance of the optic nerve; b, b, a colorless strip of that the Substance medullated nerve fibers; c, a strip of deeper color sepa- .. . rating the lighter upper from the more heavily pigmented plays an important lower portion. 2 shows the optogram of a window. part in the functional response of the rod elements. It has been shown that provision exists in the retina for the constant regeneration of this red pigment. It will be remembered that the external segments of the rods im- pinge upon the heavily pigmented epithelial cells that lie between the rods and the choroid coat. From experiments upon frogs' eyes it appears that a portion of the retina detached from the pigment 332 THE SPECIAL SENSES. cells and bleached by the action of light is not able to regenerate its visual purple until again laid back upon the choroid coat. This regenerating influence of the black pigmented cells may be con- nected with another interesting relation that they exhibit. Under normal conditions delicate processes extend from these cells and penetrate between the rods and cones. When the eye is exposed to light the black pigment migrates along these processes as far even as the external limiting membrane, and it is possible that this ar- rangement may be useful in obviating diffuse radiation of light from one rod to another. When the eye is kept in the dark, however, the pigment moves outwardly and collects around the external segments, where the process of regeneration of the visual purple is taking place. Further evidence that the visual purple is connected with the irritability of the rods toward light stimulation is shown by the fact that when it is exposed to the different rays of the spec- trum the absorption of light is greatest in that part of the spec- trum (green) which appears the brightest in vision when carried out under such conditions as may be supposed to involve the activity chiefly of the rods (see below for these conditions). It is, however, perfectly obvious that visual purple is not essential to vision. The fact that it is absent from the fovea centralis is alone sufficient proof of this statement. Moreover, it seems to be absent entirely in the eyes of some animals; for instance, the pigeon, hen, some reptiles, and some bats. The most attractive view of the function of the visual purple is that it serves to increase the delicacy of re- sponse or irritability of the rods in dim lights, — a view that is ex- plained in more detail in the paragraph below, dealing with the sup- posed difference in function between the rods and cones. The Extent of the Visual Field — Perimetry. — By the visual field of each eye is meant the entire extent of the external world which when the eye is fixed forms an image upon or is projected upon the retina of that eye. From what has been said previously regarding the dioptrics of the eye it is obvious that the visual field is inverted upon the retina, and that, therefore, objects in the upper visual field fall upon the lower half of the retina, and objects in the right half of the visual field fall upon the left, half of the retina. Since the retina is sensitive to light up to the ora serrata, it is evident that if the eye were protruded sufficiently from its orbit: its projected visual field when represented upon ;i flat surface would have the form of a circle the center of wliicli would Correspond to the fovea centralis. As ;i matter of fact, the configuration of the face is such as to cut off a considerable pari of this field and to give to the field as it actually exists an irregular outline. The bridge of the nose, the projecting eyebrows and cheek hones serve to thus limit the field. To obtain the exact outline and extent of the visual field in any given h i- only necessary to keep the eye fixed and then to move a PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 333 small object in the different meridians and at the same distance from the eye. The limits of vision may be obtained in this way along each meridian and the results combined upon an appropriate chart. An instrument, the perimeter, has been devised to facilitate the process of charting the visual field. It has been given a number of different forms, one of which is illustrated in Fig 142. The shape of the visual fields in the normal eye is represented in Fig. 143. The determination of the visual fields is of especial importance in cases of brain lesions involving the visual area in the occipital lobe. Fig. 142. — Perimeter. The semicircular bar may be placed in any meridian. A given object is then moved along the bar from without in until it is just perceived. The angular distance at which this occurs is marked off on the corresponding meridian on the chart seen at the left of the figure. The eye examined gazes over the top of the vertical rod at the right at a fixed point in the middle of the semicircular bar. The extent and portion of the retina affected may be used to aid in locating the seat of the lesion. For physiological and for clinical purposes it is necessary to distinguish between the central (or direct) and the peripheral (or indirect) fields of vision. The former term is meant to refer to that portion of the field which falls upon the fovea centralis; in other words, it is the projection, in any fixed 334 THE SPECIAL SENSES. position of the eye, of the fovea into the external world. The peripheral field refers to the rest of the visual field involving the retina outside the fovea. As a matter of fact, all of our distinct and most useful vision in the daytime at least is effected through the fovea. When the eye is kept fixed the small portion of the external world that falls upon the fovea is seen distinctly. All the rest is seen more or less indistinctly in proportion to the distance of its Fig. 14.'?. — Perimeter chart to show the field of vision for a right eye when kept in a fixed position. retinal image from the fovea. In using our eyes, therefore, we keep them continually in motion so as to bring each object, as we pay especial attention to it, in the field of central vision. The line from the fovea to the point looked at is designated as the line of sight. The area of the fovea is quite small. The measurements given by different observers vary somewhat, especially as in some the measurements are estimated for the bottom of the de- ion, the fundus, and in others for the diameter from edge to edge. The average diameter is usually given as lying between ().)! and o.l mm. bines drawn from the ends of this diameter to the nodal point of the eye subtend an angle of I degree to 1.5 degrees; and therefore all objects in the external world around the line of sight whose visual angle is within this limit, i are comprised in the central field of vision, and their retina] images fall upon the fovea,. Unilateral lesions of one occipital lobe cause half-blindness (herni- Opia) in the retinae OI] thesame side, that, is, lesions in the right PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 335 occipital lobe cause blindness of the right halves of the retinas, while injuries to the left occipital lobes are accompanied by loss of vision on the left sides of the retinas (see p. 204) ; but such unilateral lesions, it is stated, do not involve the central field of vision — only the peripheral portion of the field is affected. In connection with its special functions in vision the fovea centralis possesses a peculiar structure. It forms a shallow depression in the center of the retina described by some authors as elliptical, by others as circular in out- line. In the center of the fovea lies a smaller, very shallow depres- sion spoken of as the foveola. The diameter of the fovea, as stated above, is estimated differently by different authors. While meas- urements on preserved specimens give the diameter as 0.2 to 0.4 mm., ophthalmoscopic examination seems to indicate that in the fresh state it may be larger. According to Fritsch,* the fundus, reckoned from the point at which the depression begins, has a diam- eter of 0.5 to 0.75 mm. Within the fovea cones only are present, and these cones are longer and more slender (diameter, 0.002 mm.) than in the rest of the retina. Moreover, the thickness of the retina is much reduced in the fovea, whence arises the depression. At this point the cones are practically exposed directly to the light, whereas in other parts the light must penetrate the other layers before reaching the rods and cones. Lying around the fovea is an area about 6 mm. in diameter, of a yellowish color, and hence known as the macula lutea. According to recent observers f the yellow color of the macula is due to post-mortem changes. In a normal retina this area does not show a yellow color and there is, therefore, no reason why it should be given a special designation. Visual Acuity. — The distinctness of vision varies greatly in different parts of the retina. It is usually measured by bringing two fine lines closer and closer together until the eye is unable to see them as two distinct objects. Measured in this way it is usually stated that when the distance between the lines subtends an angle of 1 minute (60 seconds) at the eye the limit of visibility is reached. This angle on the retina comprises an area of about 0.004 mm. in diameter, sufficient to cover two cones in the fovea. A simpler method to ascertain the size of a just perceptible image on the retina is to use a black spot upon a white background. At a sufficient distance this object will be invisible, but if brought closer to the eye it will be just seen at a certain distance. The diameter of the spot being known, and its distance from the eye, the size of the retinal image may be calculated. Using this method, Guillery J estimated the size of the just perceptible retinal image, or, as it has been appropriately called, the physiological point, at 0.0035 mm. * Fritsch, "Pitzungsberichte d. konig. Akad. d. Wiss.,'' Berlin. 1900. fSiven, "Skandinaviscb.es Archiv f. Physiol.." 1905, 17, 306. t Guillery, "Zeitschrift f. Psychologie u. Physiol, d. Sinnesorgane, " 12, 243, 1896. 136 THE SPECIAL SENSES. These estimates apply only to the fovea, and, indeed, to the central part of the fovea, the foveola. Numerous authors have called attention to the fact that the size of the physiological point for the fovea varies with the intensity of illumination. The estimates given are for ordinary room light. Out of doors, and especially in the case of persons who live habitually an outdoor life, the physio- logical point is smaller — less than half the size given above. We may believe, therefore, that under the most favorable conditions It ir A' a \ \ \ \ „ .— — =*- "— —- \ s "" \ s 60° J0° W JO £0" iQ° 6° 0° <5° iO' Z0° JO 40° tffO' 60' lit'. 111. — Curve to show the relative acuity of vision in the mitral and peripheral field and in the light-adapted and 'In- dark-adapted eye. (Koeater.) Tho full line repre- i.- relative acutene of vision in the eye expo ev I lie center of t he object. This is the case, for instance, with the moon. Nevertheless, in looking ich an object as the moon the eye, to make out details, will fixate one point after another, showing thai lor most distinct PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 337 vision we use probably only the center of the fovea. As we pass out from the fovea into the peripheral field of vision the acuity of vision diminishes very rapidly, so that at 20 degrees, for instance, from the center of the fovea the physiological point on the retina is 0.035 mm.; that is, it has a diameter ten times as large as in the fovea. On this account our vision in the peripheral field is very indistinct,— details of form cannot be clearly perceived. The rapidity with which visual acuity diminishes as we pass outward from the fovea is indicated by the curve given in Fig. 144. In all close work, therefore, we keep our eyes moving continually so as to bring one point after another into the center of the fovea, as is well illustrated by the act of reading. If the eye is kept fixed upon the- central letter of a long word, only one or two letters on each side can be made out distinctly in spite of the fact that with such familiar objects we can guess the letter even when the image is not entirely distinct. In ophthalmological practice the acuity of vision (central vision) is measured usually by test letters whose size is such that at the distance at which they are read — say, 6 meters (20 feet), the practical far point at which no accommodation is needed — each subtends at the eye an angle of 5 minutes. An eye that can distinguish the letters at this distance is said to be normal ; one that can distinguish them only at a smaller distance or at the given distance requires letters of larger size has a subnormal acuity of vision. If, for instance, an individual at 20 feet can read only those letters that the normal eye can distinguish at 100 feet his visual acuity, V, is equal to -nrV Relation between the Amount of Sensation and the Intensity of the Stimulus — Threshold Stimulus. — With the sensory as with the motor nerves we may distinguish between various degrees of sub- maximal stimulation. The stronger the stimulus, the stronger the reaction, — that is, in the case of the optic nerve, the visual sensation. The end reaction of the activity of a sensory nerve is a state of con- sciousness. The variations in magnitude of this state can not be measured with objective exactness, they must be judged subjectively by the individual concerned. A stimulus too weak to give a re- sponse with a motor nerve is usually designated in physiology as subminimal ; a similar stimulus with sensory nerves is frequently expressed by the equivalent term subliminal, — that is, below the threshold. So a stimulus just strong enough to provoke a percep- tible reaction is the minimal stimulus for efferent nerves and the threshold stimulus for sensory nerves. Inasmuch as the variations in the intensity of consciousness can not be adequately measured, it is customary, in studying the relations of the strength of stimulus to the conscious response, to pay attention to the strength of stimu- lus under any given condition which is sufficient to arouse a just 22 338 THE SPECIAL SENSES. perceptible difference in the conscious reaction. Proceeding upon this method, it is found in the case of the visual sensations and the optic nerve, a^ with other sensations and their corresponding nerves, that the increase of stimulus necessary to cause a just perceptible change in consciousness varies with the amount of stimulus already acting. If, for instance, the retina is being stimulated by a light of 1 candle power an increase of illumination to 1.1 candle power may make a perceptible difference in sensation. But if the retina is being illuminated by a light of 10 candle power an increase to 10.1 candle power would probably make no perceptible difference. For a certain range of stimulation, in fact, it has been stated that the increase in stimulus must be a constant fractional part of the stimu- lus already acting. That is, in the hypothetical case given, if, with 1 candle power, an increase to 1.1 candle power makes a just per- ceptible difference in consciousness, then with 10 candle power an increase of -^ of the acting stimulus, namely — 1 candle power — will be necessary to cause a perceptible difference. The relation as expressed in this form is known as Weber's law; but it seems prob- able that, while the general fact is true, tins exact expression of it holds only approximately for an intermediate range of stimulation. In this matter of a threshold stimulus the sensitiveness of the retina shows also certain interesting differences in the foveal as compared with the peripheral field. The difference is especially marked when the reaction of the retina in strong lights is compared with its reaction in dim lights. The Light-adapted and the Dark-adapted Eye. — The con- dition of the retina changes when after exposure to light it is sub- mitted to darkness, the change being most marked in the peripheral field. When one passes from daylight into a dark room vision at first is very imperfect, but after some minutes it rapidly im- proves, "as the eye becomes accustomed to the dark." The change is known as an adaptation, and in this respect the retina differs from the sensitive photographic plate. Comparison of the threshold stimulus for different parts of the retina, in an eye exposed alternately to darkness and to light, lias shown that. in the dark the sensitiveness in the peripheral field increases greatly during an hour or BO, while that of the foveal held is appar- ently unchanged. With such a dark-adapted eye. therefore, there will he a certain dim lighl which will he seen by the per- ipheral parts of the retina, hut perhaps will cause no reaction upon the fovea. For such a degree of light, therefore, the fovea would he blind. This general fact has, indeed, long been known. Anyone may notice in late twilight, when the stars are beginning to appear, thai a very faint star may disappear when looked at,— that 18, When it - image U brought upon t he fovea ;tO see it one must direct, little to 1 be side, so as to bring its image into the periph- PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 339 eral field. This greater sensitiveness of the dark-adapted eye in the peripheral field where the rods predominate over the cones seems to be associated with the movement of the pigment in the pigment epithelium (see above) and the resulting regeneration of the visual purple in the external segments of the rods. The increase in the visual purple in the dark may, indeed, account for the increased sensitiveness to light in the rod-region and explain why a similar increase fails to occur in the fovea, where only cones are present. The curve given in Fig. 144 shows that in the dark-adapted eye the acuity of vision in the peripheral field is greater than in the fovea. In accordance with these facts von Kries* has suggested that the rods, the peripheral field of the retina, are especially adapted for vision in dim lights, night vision, while the cones are especially adapted for vision in strong lights, day vision. This general fact will perhaps accord with the experience of anyone who attempts to estimate the value of his peripheral vision in dim nightlight as compared with daylight. Other interesting differences in the reac- tion of the light-adapted and the dark-adapted eye are referred to below in connection with color blindness. CHARACTERISTICS OF THE VISUAL SENSATIONS. In addition to the spatial attributes connected with our visual sensations — that is, the perception of form — they are characterized by two properties which may be described in general as variations in intensity and in quality. Luminosity or Brightness. — That characteristic which we describe as the luminosity or brightness of a visual sensation has been defined differently by various writers. We may consider it, however, as the expression of the intensity of the acting stimulus. Sensations of the same quality are easily compared as regards their brightness. We can tell as between two whites or two greens which is the brighter of the two, but when two different qualities — a red and a green sensation, for instance — are compared our subjective determination of the relative brightness is, for most persons, difficult or impossible to make. To a lesser degree the difficulty is similar to that of the comparison of sight and sound. According to the conception adopted here, however, that the brightness is an ex- pression of the intensity of the stimulus, an objective standard of comparison might be obtained by measuring the resulting action cur- rents in the optic nerve fibers. When the spectral colors are ex- amined it is obvious that some of the colors are brighter than others, the extreme red and extreme violet, for instance, possessing little luminosity as compared with the yellow. The relative brightness * Von Kries, "Zeitschrift f. Psychologie u. Phvsiologie d. Sinnesorgane," 9, 81, 1895. 340 THE SPECIAL SENSES. of the different spectral colors is found to vary with the amount of illumination, as shown in the curves given in Fig. 145. With a brilliant spectrum the maximum brightness is in the yellow, but with a feeble illumination it shifts to the green. This fact accords with what is known as the " Purkinje phenomenon," — namely, the changing luminosity and color value of colors in dim lights. As the light becomes more feeble the colore toward the red end of the spectrum lose their quality, the blue colors being perceived last of all, just as in late twilight it may be noticed that the sky remains distinctly blue after the colors of the landscape become indistin- guishable. It should be added that the " Purkinje phenomenon " is true only for the parts of the retina lying outside the fovea, that is, for the peripheral field. As the light grows dimmer the perception of blue is lost first in the fovea, so that with a certain 3.0 y, **■ Sr- tfi- *A V, U\ %r i» tfi **\ a /,- OH o.c OA /' . / / / / / / J / / / / / / 1 1 ! i i l \ V i i \ \ • i 1 > i i l ■ i \ > A \\ i i i I \ \ It i — — i ^A l! a t ~ z>'i'** Hfr'<*v". t i I , VN / r,'- / i ,'• ^■■ ,.„! i «■><> oto tas eot sso sis sss as sto sos tvo *w wo *s BCD E £ C Fig, 145 . — Diagram showing the distribution of the intensity of the spectrum as de- pendent upon the degree of illumination. The spectrum is represented along the abscissa, the numerals giving the wave length from red, 070, to violet, •):;<>. The ordinates give the lumino ity of the different colors. Bight curves are given to show the changes in di tnbunpp of relative bnghtnei i with changes in degree <>f illumination. With the i illumination the maximum brightness is in the yellow (606-625); with weaker illumination it shifts to the green (o :',.">).— (Kiiniq. ) feebleness of illumination the central field becomes blue-blind. With a very feeble illumination the dark-adapted eye becomes practically totally color blind. Qualities of Visual Sensations. -The different qualities of our color sensations may be arranged in two scries: an achromatic PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 341 series, consisting of white and black and the intermediate grays, and a chromatic series, comprising the various spectral colors, together with the purples made by combination of the two ends of the spectrum, red and blue, and the colors obtained by fusion of the spectral colors with white or with black, such, for instance, as the olives and browns. The Achromatic Series. — Our standard white sensation is that caused by sunlight. Objects reflecting to our eye all the visible rays of the sunlight give us a white sensation. This sensation, therefore, is due primarily to the combined action of all the visible rays of the spectrum, each of which, taken separately, would give us a color sensation. White or gray may be produced also by the combined action of certain pairs of colors, — complementary colors, — as is described below. Black, on the contrary, is the sensation caused by withdrawal of light. It must be emphasized that in order to see black a retina must be present. It is probable that a person with both eyes enucleated has no sensation of darkness. That black is a sensation referable to a condition of the retina is made probable also by the interesting observations recorded by Gotch,* — namely, that when an eye that has been exposed to light is suddenly cut off from the light there is an electrical change in the retina, a dark response, similar to that caused by throwing light on a retina previously kept in the dark. Blackness, therefore, is a sensation produced by withdrawing light from the retina, and a black object is one that reflects no light to the eye. Black may be combined with white to produce the series of grays, and when com- bined with the spectral colors it gives a series of modified color tones, thus the olives of different shades may be considered as combina- tions of green and black in varying proportions. The chromatic series consists of those qualities to which we give the name of colors, and, as stated above, they comprse the spectral colors, and the extraspectral color, purple, together with the light- weak and light-strong hues obtained by combining the colors with white or black. In the spectrum many different colors may be detected, — some observers record as many as one hundred and sixty, — but in general we give specific names only to those that stand sufficiently far apart to represent quite distinct sensations, — namely, the red, orange, yellow, green, blue, and violet. When light is taken from a definite limited portion of the spectrum we have a monochromatic light that gives us a distinct color sensation varying with the wave length of the portion chosen. Color Saturation and Color Fusion. — The term saturation as applied to colors is meant to define their freedom from accompany- ing white sensation. A perfectly saturated color would be one entirely free from mixture with white. On the objective side it is * Gotch, " Journal of Physiology," 29, 388, 1903. 342 THE SPECIAL SENSES. easy to select a monochromatic bundle of rays from the spectrum without admixture of white light, but on the physiological side it is not probable that the color sensat ion thus produced is entirely free from white sensation, since the monochromatic rays may initiate in the retina not only the specific processes underlying the pro- duction of its special color, but at the same time give rise in some degree to the processes causing white sensations. Even the spectral colors are therefore not entirely saturated, but they come as near to giving us this condition as we can get without changing the state of the retina itself by previous stimulation. Color Fusion. — By color fusion we mean the combination of two or more color processes in the retina, this end being obtained by superposing upon the same portion of the retina the rays giving rise to these color processes. It must be borne in mind that color fusion upon the retina is quite a different thing -from color mixture as practised by the artist. A blue pigment, such as Prussian blue, for instance, owes its blue color to the fact that when sunlight falls upon it the red-yellow rays are absorbed and only the blue, with some of the green, rays are reflected to the eye. So a yellow pig- ment, chrome yellow, absorbs the blue, violet, and red rays and reflects to the eye only the yellow with some of the green rays. A mixture of the two upon the palette will absorb all the rays except the green and will therefore appear green to the eye. If, however, by means of a suitable device, we throw simultaneously upon the retina a blue and a yellow light, the result of the retinal fusion is a sensation of white. Many different methods have been employed to throw colors simultaneously upon the retina, the most perfect being a system of lenses or mirrors by which different portions of a spectrum can be superposed. The usual device employed in laboratory experiments is that of rotation of discs of colored paper. Each disc has a slit in it from center to periphery so that two discs can be fitted together to expose more or less of each color. If a combination of this kind is attached to a small electrical motor it can be rotated so rapidly that the impressions of the two colors upon I he ict ina follow at such a short, interval of time as to be prac- tically simultaneous. The Fundamental Colors. -By the methods of color fusion il ran be shown that three colors may he selected from the spec- trum whose combinations in different proportions will give white, or any of the intermediate color shades, or purple Considered purely objectively, a Bel of three such colors may be designated as the fundamental color and red, yellow, and blue, or red, green, and violet have been the three colors selected. On the LOlogical side, however, it has been assumed that, there are certain more or less independent color processes photochemical processes in the retina which give us our fundamental color sen- PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 343 sations, and that all other color sensations are combinations of these processes in varying proportions with each other or with the proc- esses causing white and black. Referring only to the colors proper, the fundamental color sensations according to some views are red, green, and blue or violet; according to others, they are red, yellow, green, and blue. (See paragraph on Theories of Color Vision.) Helmholtz calls attention to the fact that the names used for these funda- mental color sensations are obviously of ancient origin, thus indicating that the difference in quality of the sensations has been long recognized. Red is from the Sanskrit rudhira, blood; blue from the same root as blow, and re- fers to the color of the air ; green from the same root as grow, referring to the color of vegetation. Yellow seems to be derived from the same root as gold, which typified the color. The other less distinct qualities have names of recent application, such as orange, violet, indigo blue, etc. Complementary Colors. — It has been found by the methods of color fusion that certain pairs of colors when combined give a white (gray) sensation. It may be said, in fact, that for any given color there exists a complement such that the fusion of the two in suitable proportions gives white. If we confine ourselves to the spectral colors we recognize such complementary pairs as the following: Red and greenish blue. Orange and cyan blue. Yellow and indigo blue. Greenish yellow and violet. The complementary color for green is the extraspectral purple. Colors that are closer together in the spectral series than the complementaries give on fusion some intermediate color which is more saturated — that is, less mixed with white sensation — the nearer the colors are together. Thus, red and yellow, when fused, give orange. Colors farther apart than the distance of the comple- mentaries give some shade of purple. On the physical side, there- fore, we can produce a sensation of white in two ways : Either by the combined action of all the visible rays of the spectrum (sunlight) or by the combined action of pairs of colors whose wave lengths vary by a certain interval. It is probable that in the retina the processes induced by these two methods are qualitatively the same, the wave-lengths represented by the complementary colors setting up by their combined action the same photochemical processes that normally are induced by the sunlight. After-images. — As the name implies, this term refers to images that remain in consciousness after the objective stimulus has ceased to act upon the retina. They are due doubtless to the fact that the changes set up in the retina by the visual stimulus continue, with or without modification, after the stimulus is withdrawn. After- images are of two kinds: positive and negative. In the positive after-images the visual sensation retains its normal colors. If one 344 THE SPECIAL SENSES. looks at an incandescent electric light for a few seconds and then closes his eyes he continues to see the luminous object for a con- siderable time in its normal colors. Objects of much less inten- sity of illumination may give positive after-images, especialy when the eyes have been kept closed for some time, as, for instance, upon waking in the morning. In negative after- images the colors are all reversed — that is, they take on the complementary qualities (see Fig. 146). 'White becomes black, red, a bluish green, and vice versa. Negative after-images are produced very easily by fixing the eyes steadily upon a given object for an interval of twenty seconds or more and then closing them. In the case of colored objects the after- image is shown better, perhaps, by turning the eyes upon a white surface after the period of fixation is over. After-images produced in this way often appear and disappear a number of times before ceasing entirely, and, although the color at first is the complementary' of that of the object looked at, it may change before it- final disappearance. Anyone who has gazed for even a brief interval at the setting sun will remember the number of colored and changing after-images seen for a time when the eye is turned to another portion of the sky. That several different after-images are seen in this case is due to the fact that the eyes are not kept fixed under the dazzling light of the sun, and a number of different images are formed, therefore, upon the retina. After-images may be used in a very instructive way to showr that our estimates of the size of a retinal image vary with the distance to which we project it, — that is, with the distance at which we suppose we see it. Once the image is, so to speak, branded on the retina, its actual size, of course, does not vary, but our judgment of its size may be made to vary rapidly by projecting the image upon screens at different distances. If, for instance, in obtaining the after-image of the strips diown in Fig. 146 one moves the white paper used to catch t he image toward and away from the eye, the apparent size varies proportionally to its distance. Color Contrasts.— By color contrast is meant the influence that one color field has upon a contiguous one. If, for instance, a piece of blue paper is laid upon a, larger yellow square, the color of each of them is heightened by contrast. A piece of blue paper on a blue background does not appear so saturated as when placed :iLr.-iin-i :i yellow background. The influences of contrasl may tie shown in a greal variety of ways.* for instance, if a disc; like thai in the illustration, Fig. I ix.i, b rotated rapidly, it should , the darke I ;it the middle; but each circle should he uniform ;i ■ it i- made by the fusion of a definite amount of white and black. ' >B t tie conl rary, t he appearance obtained is that repre ■ Rood, " Modem Chromatics," " International Scientific Series." << c s-s-o < g » 3,2 » o S p .-"p^cs^bijo 3"„ 3 -1 a ^ 5 ;;•» w ^ o ^^ 8-™ 9-3 ? 3 CD C a, 3.™ p,0 CD o GO _ _ a p o CD 0 E:2 3<^2 Q tD 0 p — — — to CD 5 ■cd > p O 3 — CD 13 e* - o 2 3" CD •a K - d CD o O ,B t n | o o' p a a u 5' — — 2- 5 — CD 2. S" 2 a 5. Q. 5 ; c p CD P r (5 w 3 z _ 3 o CD O a CD -i 3" CD CD W P 0 5 p CD CD O d CO < CD P CD CD tc CD " p^ - S.« 5 » 5 & s "^ 2- ^ P* 'B CD hCTj. CD k-.CD f-^CD j^ 3 st£LH-2 B"!? 3 £~,b cd B-^C P m m "< B * p „ S ?3m' * s-2- O 3-2 5- 3. R cr~a.P"o a" CD _ o. - ° B-o-Ea. R ffi- P>C, G «■ CD £ *S T (S Q r * PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 345 sented in Fig. 1485. Each circle appears darker on its outer edge where it borders on a lighter circle, and lighter on its inner edge where it borders on a darker circle. Similar contrasts may be obtained from comparing shadows cast by yellow and white light. If a rod be arranged in a dark room so as to cast a shadow from an opening admitting daylight and one also from a lighted candle, either shadow taken separately appears black, but if the two are cast side by side one will appear blue, the other yellow. The shadow cast by the daylight, being illuminated by the yellow candle-light, will appear yellow, and the other shadow, that from the candle-light, will by contrast seem quite blue. A striking instance of the effect of con- trast is given, also, by the simple experiment of Mayer, illustrated in Fig. 147. The gray square on the green background suffers no apparent change from contrast, but if the figure is covered by a sheet of white tissue paper the gray square at once takes on a red- dish hue. It is evident that in all artistic and ornamental employ- ment of colors this influence must be considered, and empirical rules are established which indicate for the normal eye the bene- Fig. 148A — Black and white disc for ex- periment on contrast. — {Rood.) Fig. 148B. — Showing the result when the disc A. is set into rapid rotation. — (Rood.) ficial or the killing effect of different colors when brought into juxtaposition. Color Blindness. — The fact that some eyes do not possess normal color vision does not seem to have attracted the attention of scientific observers until it was studied with some care by Dalton, the distinguished English chemist, at the end of the eighteenth century. Dalton himself suffered from color blindness, and the particular variety exhibited by him was for some time described as Daltonism, but is now usually designated as red blindness. The subject was given practical importance by later observers, espe- cially by the Swedish physiologist Holmgren,* who emphasized its relations to possible accidents by rail or at sea in connection with * Holmgren, "Color Blindness in its Relations to Accidents by Rail and Sea," " Smithsonian Institution Reports," Washington, 1878. See also Jeffries, "Color Blindness, its Dangers and its Detection," Boston. 3-46 THE SPECIAL SENSES. colored signals. It is now the practice in all civilized countries to require tests for color blindness in the case of those who in railways or upon vessels may be responsible for the interpretation of signals. The numerous statistics that have been gathered show that the defect is fairly prevalent, especially among men. It is said that on the average from 2 to 4 per cent, are color blind among males, while among women the proportion is much smaller, — 0.01 to 1 per cent. Among the poorly educated classes the defect is said to be more common than among educated persons. Color blindness may exist in different degrees of completeness, from a total loss to a simple imperfection or feebleness of the color sense, and it is usually congenital. Those who are completely color blind as re- gards some or all of the fundamental colors fall into two groups: the dichromatic, whose color vision may be represented by two fundamental colors and their combinations with white or black, and the monochromatic or totally color blind, who see only the white-gray-black series. Dichromatic Vision. — The color-blind who belong to this class fall into two or three groups, which have been designated, under the influence of the Young-He lmholtz theory of color vision, the red-blind, the green-blind, and the violet-blind. As the terms red-blind and green-blind imply a more specific condition of vision than is found to be the case on careful examination, von Kries has suggested as a substitute the names protanopia and deuteranopia, as indicating a defect in a first or second constit- uent necessary for color vision. According to the same nomen- clature so-called violet-blindness would be designated as tritanopia. The most common by far of these groups is that of so-called red- blindness (protanopia); it constitutes the usual form of color blindness. As a matter of fact, persons so affected are in reality red-green blind. In what may be called the most typical cases they distinguish in the spectrum only yellows and blues. The red, orange, yellow, and green appear as yellow of different shades, the green-blue as gray, and the blue-violet and purple as blue. The red end of the spectrum is distinctly shortened, especially if the illumination is poor, and the maximum luminosity, instead of being in the yellow, as in normal eyes, is in the green. When the spectrum is examined by such persons a neutral gray band is geen a1 the junction of the blue and green. In some cases, how- ever, this neutral band is not seen, the yellow passing with but little change into the blue. As a matter of fact, in red-blindness the most Characteristic defect is a failure to see or to appreciate the green. This color is confused with the grays arid with dull Shades Of red Wnen SUCh persons are examined for their negative after-images for different colors, it will be noted thai they de- PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 347 scribe some of their after-images as red, the after-image of indigo- blue, for example, but that they describe none as green. The after-image of purple, for instance, which to the normal eye is bright green, is described by them as gray blue or pale blue. From the descriptions given it is probable that the color vision of the so-called red-blind is not by any means the same in all cases, but exhibits many individual differences. The green-blind are also, according to recent descriptions, red-green blind; they also confuse reds and greens and in the spectrum are conscious of only two color qualities, namely, yellow and blue. They differ from the red-blind in that the red end of the spectrum is not shortened, and the maximum luminosity, as with the normal eye, is placed in the yellow. In the matching and combination of colors they show distinct differences from the red-blind, so that though re- sembling the latter in general features, they differ obviously in some details. Violet blindness (tritanopia) seems to be so rare as a congenital and permanent condition that no very exact study of it has been made. In cases of acquired tritanopia resulting from pathological changes it is reported that the violet end of the spectrum is colorless (neutral) and that a neutral band appears also in the yellow-green region of the spectrum.* By the ingestion of santonin it is said that a condition of this kind may be produced temporarily. The violet end of the spectrum is shortened and white objects take on a yellowish hue. The conditions produced by santonin are evidently more complex than can be explained by simply assuming that the violet color sense is lost. Recent observers f state that the drug produces a condition of yellow vision, outside the fovea, in the daylight, and a condition of violet vision with yellow-blindness, but no red- or green-blindness, in dim lights. Tests for Color Blindness. — Although the vision of the red and the green blind is deficient as regards green and red colors, it will be found in many cases that they recognize these colors and name them correctly, having adopted the usual nomenclature and adapted it to their own standards. In order to detect the deficiency they must be examined by some test which will compel them to match certain colors. Under these circumstances it will be found that along with correct matches they will make others which to the nor- mal eye are entirely erroneous. A great number of methods have been proposed and used to detect color blindness. The simplest perhaps is that of Holmgren. J A number of skeins of wool are used * Collins and Nagel, " Zeitschrift f. Psychol, u. Physiol, d. Sinnesorgane," 1906, xli., 74. fSiven and Wendt, " Skandinavisches Archiv f. Physiologie," 14, 196, 1903, and 1905, 17, 306. % For details see the works of Holmgren and of Jeffries, already quoted. 348 THE SPECIAL SENSES. and three test colors are chosen, — namely, (1) a pale pure green skein, which must not incline toward yellow green; (II) a medium purple (magenta) skein; and (III) a vivid red skein. The person under investigation is given skein I and is asked to select from the pile of assorted colored skeins those that have a similar color value. He is not to make an exact match, but to select those that appear to have the same color. Those who are red or green blind will see the test skein as a gray with some yellow or blue shade and will select, therefore, not only the green skeins, but the grays or grayish yellow and blue skeins. To ascertain whether the individual is red or green blind tests II and III may then be employed. With test II, medium purple, the red blind will select, in addition to other purples, only blues or violets; the green blind will select as "confusion colors" only greens and grays. With test III, red, the red blind will select as confusion colors greens, grays, or browns less luminous than the test color, while the green blind will select greens, grays, or browns of a greater brightness than the test. Monochromatic Vision. — A number of cases of total color blindness have been carefully examined.* It would seem that in such individuals there is an entire loss of color sense,- — they possess only achromatic vision. The external world appears to them only in shades of gray. In the majority of these cases (-f) there is a region of blindness in the fovea (central scotoma), and an unusual sensitiveness to light and nystagmus (rolling movement of the eyeballs) are also characteristic. Since the peripheral field of vision is nearly normal as regards sensitiveness to light, while the central (ield is frequently blind or amblyopic, it has been assumeu that this condition is one of loss of function in the cones. Distribution of the Color Sense in the Retina. — What has been said above in regard to color blindness refers especially to the central field of vision. When we examine the peripheral field in the normal eye it is found that on the extreme periphery (lie retina i- totally color blind, perceiving only light and darkness, — that is, the h.nli - of gray. As we pass in toward the center the color Bense develops gradually, the blue colors being perceived first and the greens last,- that is, nearest to the center, — so that in a cer- tain /one the normal eye is red-green blind. The distribution of the color sen <■ may be studied conveniently by means of the pe- limeter (see \>. 332). It will l>e found to vary with each individual, so much so thai it is possible that a test of this character might he used for the identification of individuals. Exceptionally if is found that the entire retina possesses ;i nearly normal color sense. Usu- •"Gninert, "Archiv flir Ophthalmologic," 56, 132, L903. PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 349 ally, for the colors red, green, and blue, the blue has the most exten- sive field and the green the least, as is indicated in the perimeter chart given in Fig. 149. If the green chosen is blue green (490/*/*) — that is, the complementary of the red — it is stated that their fields are co-extensive.* From this standpoint the retina presents three concentric zones: an extreme peripheral zone devoid of color vision, an intermediate zone in which yellow and blue are perceived, and a central zone sensitive to red and green. The outlines of 061 081 O^-1 Fig. 149. — Perimeter chart indicating the average fields of vision for blir;, red, and green compared with white (gray). Right eye: The outlines of the color fields are repre- sented as smooth since the chart is an average from many determinations. As a matter of fact, in each individual the outline is highly irregular. Normally green (bright green) is the smallest field, green objects outside the limit appearing yellow and farther out colorless (gray). the different fields usually show many irregularities, and in some cases it will be found that bright green is perceived over a larger area than the red. The fields are not identical in the two eyes, and in each eye it is, as a rule, more extensive upon the nasal than upon the temporal side of the retina. In the red-green blind the peripheral fields of color vision, judged by the individual's own standards, may be markedly constricted as compared with the nor- mal retina (see Fig. 150). Functions of the Rods and Cones. — Many facts unite in mak- ing it probable that the rods and cones are different in function. * Baird, "The Color Sensitivity of the Peripheral Retina," Carnegie Pub- lication, No. 29, 1905. 350 THE SPECIAL SENSES. They differ in structure ami especially in their connections. As is shown in the diagram given in Fig. 151, the cones terminate in the external nuclear layer in arborizations which connect with the bipolar ganglion cells, and in the fovea at least this connection is such that each cone connects with a single nerve cell and eventually per- haps with a single optic nerve fiber. The rods, on the contrary, end in a single knob-like swelling, and a number of them make con- nections with the same nerve cell. Histologically, therefore, the 06i 081 oLX 09V Fig. 150. — Perimeter chart showing the highly restricted color fields in the left eyo of a typical ca e •■! o-called red-green color blindness. The ability to distinguish red and green, by whatever characteristics of intensity or color they possessed extended for a very snort distance outside the fovea. It is interesting that the ability to distinguish blue was in this case limited as compared with a normal eye. conduction paths lor the cones seem to be more direct than in the case of the rods. These latter elements, moreover, possessthe visual purple, which is lacking in the cones. Lastly, in the eye of i lie totally color blind, in the dark-adapted eye in dim lights, in the color-blind peripheral area of the normal eye, and in the eyes of mot distinctly night-seeing animals, such as the mole and the owl, vision eemi to be effected solely by the rods. These facts find their Bimplesl explanation perhaps in the view advocated by J'ari- naud, I ranklin, von Kries,* and others, according to which the perception of color IS a function of the cones alone, while the rods + Von Kries, "Zeil chrifl i. Psychologic u, Physiol, d. Sinnesorgane," it, SI, IS'.).',. PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 351 are sensitive only to light and darkness, and by virtue of their power of adaptation in the dark through the regeneration of their visual purple they form also the special apparatus for vision in dim lights (night vision). Color blindness, therefore, whether total or partial, may be regarded as an affection or lack of normal develop- ment of the cones. On the other hand, those interesting cases in B Fig. 151. — Schema of the structure of the human retina (Greeff): I, Pigment layer; II, rod and cone layer; ///, outer nuclear layer; IV, external plexiform layer; V, layer of horizontal cells; VI, layer of bipolar cells (inner nuclear) ; VII, layer of amacrmal cells (without axons); VIII, inner plexiform layer; IX, ganglion cell layer; X, nerve fiber layer; 6, fiber of Mtiller. which the vision, while good in daylight, is faulty or lacking in dim lights (night blindness, hemeralopia) may be referred to a defective functional activity of the rods, probabby from lack of formation of visual purple. 352 THE SPECIAL SENSES. Theories of Color Vision. — A number of theories have been proposed to explain the facts of color vision. None of them has been entirely successful in the sense that the explanations it affords have been submitted to satisfactory experimental verification. The immediate stimuli that give rise to the visual impulses are assumed to be of a chemical nature, and it seems probable that in this case as in that of many other problems of physiology, we must await the development of a more complete knowledge of the chemical processes involved. The theories proposed at present, while all tested by experimental inquiries, are in a large measure hypotheses constructed to fit more or less completely the facts that are known. Three of these theories may be described briefly as examples of the modes of reasoning employed: I. The Yomig- Helmholtz Theory. — This theory, proposed essen- tially by Thomas Young (1807) and afterward modified and ex- panded by Helmholtz* rests upon the assumption that there are three fundamental color sensations, — red, green, and violet — and corresponding with these there are three photochemical substances in the retina. By the decomposition of each of these substances cor- responding nerve fibers are stimulated and impulses are conducted to a special system of nerve cells in the visual center of the cerebrum. The theory, therefore, assumes special nerve fibers and nerve centers corresponding respectively to the red, green, and violet photo- chemical substances, and the peculiar quality of the resulting sensa- tions are referred, in the original theory, to the different reactions in consciousness in the three corresponding centers in the brain. When these three substances are equally excited a sensation of white results, of greater or less intensity according to the extent of the excitation. White, therefore, on this theory, is a compound sensation produced by the combination or fusion in consciousness of the three equal fundamental color sensations. The sensation of black, on the other hand, results from the absence of stimulation, from the condition of rest in the retina and in the corresponding aerve fibers and nerve centers. All other color sensations — yellow, for instance — are compound sensations produced by the combined stimulation of the three photochemical substances in different propor- tions. It is assumed, furthermore, that each of the photochemical substances is acted upon more or less by all of the visible rays of the spectrum, but that the rays of long wave lengths at the red end of the spectrum affect chiefly the red substance, those corresponding to the green of the spectrum chiefly the green substance, and the hortesl wavelength chiefly the violet substance. These rela- tionships are expressed in the diagram given in Fig. 152. The figure also indicates that it is impossible to stimulate any one of these sub- * Helmholtz, "Handbuch der pliysiolotfischen Optik," second edition, JS9G, [,344. PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 353 stances entirely alone, — that is, we cannot obtain a perfectly satu- rated color sensation. Even the extreme red or the extreme violet rays act more or less on all of the substances, and the resulting red or violet sensation, is, therefore, mixed to some extent with white, — that is, is not entirely saturated. The theory, as stated by Helm- holtz, held strictly to the doctrine of specific nerve energy, in assuming that each photochemical substance serves simply as a means for the excitation of a nerve fiber, and that the quality of the sensation aroused depends on the ending of this fiber in the brain. The phe- nomenon of negative after-images finds a simple explanation in terms of this theory. If we look fixedly at a green object, for example, the corresponding photochemical substance is chiefly acted upon, and if subsequently the same part of the retina is exposed to white light, the red and violet substances, having been previously less acted upon, now respond in greater proportions to the white light, and Fig. 152. — Schema to illustrate the Young-Helmholtz theory of color vision. — (Helm- holtz.) The spectral colors are arranged in their natural order, — red to violet. The curves represent the intensity of stimulation of the three color substances: 1, The red perceiving substance; 2, the green perceiving; 3, the. violet perceiving. Verticals drawn at any point of the spectrum indicate the relative amount of stimulation of the three substances for that wave length of the spectrum. the after-image takes a red- violet — that is, purple — color. Many objections have been raised to the Young-Helmholtz theory. It has been urged, for instance, that we are not conscious that white or yellow sensations are blends or compounded color sensations; we perceive in them none of the supposed component elements as we do in such undoubted mixtures as the blue-greens or the purples. The theory explains poorly or not at all the fact that on the periphery of the retina we are color blind and yet can perceive white or gray, and it breaks down also in the face of the facts of partial and com- plete color blindness. The explanation given for black is also unsatisfactory in that it assumes an active state of consciousness associated with a condition of rest in the visual mechanism. II. Hering's Theory of Color Vision. — This theory also assumes the existence in the retina of three photochemical substances, but 23 354 THE SPECIAL SENSES. of such a nature as to give us six different qualities of sensation. There is a white-black substance which when acted upon by the visible rays of light undergoes disassimilation and sets up nerve impulses that arouse in the brain the sensation of white. On the other hand, when not acted upon by light this same substance under- goes assdmilatory processes that in turn set up nerve impulses which in the brain give us a sensation of black. There are in the retina also a red-green and a yellow-blue substance. The former when acted upon by the longer rays undergoes disassimilation and gives a sensation of red, while the shorter waves cause assimilation and produce a sensation of green. A similar assumption is made for the yellow-blue substance. The essence of the theory may be stated, therefore, in tabular form, as follows *: Photochemical Substance. Red-green Retin'al Process. Disassimilation = 1 Assimilation Tr „ , , f Disassimilation Yellow-blue \ Assimilation White-black. f Disassimilation \ Assimilation Sensation. red green yellow blue white black It will be observed that the theory gives an independent ob- jective cause for the sensations of white, black, and yellow, and in i L63. Schema to illu trate the Hcrinc theory of color vision. — (Foster.) The curves indicate the relative inten itie "i timulation ol the three color i ubstanoes by dif« ferenl part "I the pectrum. Ordinates above the axis, X—X, indicate oatabolic chances imilation), those below anabolic changes (assimilation). Curve a represents the conditioni for ih<- black-white substance. Ii is stimulated by all the rays <>l the visible spectrum with maximum inten ity in the yellow. Curve c representi the red-green sub- stance, the longer wave lengths causing disassimilation (red), the horter ones assimilation (greei mditions for the yellow-blue substance. this respect satisfies the objections made on this score to the Young- Helmholtz theory. It fits better, also, the facts of partial and total color blindness. In the latter condition one may assume, in terms of * For discussion of color theories see Calkins, "Archiv f. Physiologic," L902, uppl. volume, p, 2 1 1. PROPERTIES OF THE RETINA. 355 v.„.y w this theory, that only the white-black substance is present, while red and green blindness — both of them, it will be recalled, really forms of red-green blindness — are explained on the view that in such persons the red-green substance is deficient or lacking. On this theory, complementary colors — red and blue-green, yellow and blue — are, in reality, antagonistic colors. When thrown on the retina simultaneously their effects neutralize each other, and there remains over only the disassimilatory effect on the white substance which is exerted by all the visible rays. The effect of the vari- ous visible rays of the spec- trum on the three photo- chemical substances is illus- trated by the chart given in Fig. 153. Ordinates above the abscissa representing dis- assimilatory effects; those below, assimilatory. III. The Franklin Theory of Color Vision (Molecular Dissociation Theory) . — This theory, proposed by Mrs. C. L. Franklin,* takes into ac- count the fact of a gradual evolution of the color sense of the retina from a primitive condition of colorless vision such as still exists in the periphery of the retina and in the eyes of the totally color blind. It assumes that the colorless sensations — white, gray, black — are occa- sioned by the reactions of a photochemical material which for convenience may be designated as the gray substance. This substance in the normal eye exists in both rods and cones; in the latter, however, in a differentiated condition capable of giving color sensations. When the molecules of this substance are completely dissociated by the action of light, gray * Franklin, " Zeitschrift f. Psychologie und Phys. d. Sinnesorgane , " 1892, iv; also "Mind," 2, 473, 1893, and " Psychological Review," 1894, 1896, 1899. Ji Fig. 154. — Schema to illustrate the Frank- lin theory of color vision (Franklin) : W, The molecule of the primitive visual (gray-perceiv- ing) substance; Y and B, the first step in the differentiation into a yellow- and a blue-per- ceiving substance, whose combined dissociation gives the same effect as that of the original sub- stance, W; G and R, the second step in the differentiation of the yellow-perceiving sub- stance, the combined dissociation of the two giving the same effect as that of the yellow-per- ceiving substance alone. The complete devel- opment of color vision as it exists. in the central part of the retina consists in the existence of three substances, which, taken separately, give red, green, and blue color sensations. 356 THE SPECIAL SENSES. sensations result, and as this is the only reaction possible in the rods these elements can furnish us only sensations of this quality. The molecules of gray substance in the cones, on the other hand, have undergone a development such that certain portions only of the molecule may become dissociated by the action of light of certain periods of vibration. This development may be sup- posed to have taken place in two stages: first, the i'oimation of two groupings within the molecule, one of which is dissociated by the slower waves and gives a sensation of yellow, and one of which is dissociated by the more rapid waves and gives the sensation of blue. This stage remains still on portions of the periphery of the retina, and is the condition present in the fovea also in the eyes of the red-green blind. The second stage consists in the division of the yellow component into two additional groupings in one of which the atomic movements are of such a period as to be affected by the longest visible waves, the red of the spectrum, while the other is dissociated by rays corresponding to the green of the spectrum and gives rise to the sensation of green. If the red and green groupings are dissociated together the resulting effect is the same as follows from the dissociation of the entire yellow component, while the complete dissociation of the red, green, and blue groupings gives the stimulus obtained originally from the disso- ciation of the whole molecule, and causes gray sensations. The idea of this subdivision or differentiation in structure of the original gray substance is indicated diagrammatically in Fig. 154. The theory accounts admirably for many phenomena in vision, and is perhaps esp cially adapted to explain the facts of color blindness and the variations in quality of our visual sensations in the peripheral areas of the retina. An extension and modification of this theory has been published by Schenck.* He assumes that each of the three-color perceiving substances is composed of two parts. One part which acts as a receiver for the stimulus, a sort of an optical resonator, in fact, and a second part which is set into activity by the receiver and gives rise to the corresponding color sensation. The theory is very elastic in its adaptability to the various kinds of color blindness. The two latter theories seem to imply thai a number of different kinds of impulses may be transmitted along the optic fibers. Bering's theory re- quiree apparently the possibility <>!' six qualitatively different impulses, — namely, while, black, red, green, yellow, ana blue, while the Franklin theory ee impulse com ponding i<> white (gray), red, green, yellow, and blue. Black i not specifically accounted for except ae a part of the gray series. At i in physiology there is n<> proof that nerve impulses can differ quali- tatively from each other, although it may be urged, perhaps with equal Force, that there is no proof that they can not so differ. The doctrine of specific nerve energy a tunes thai nerve impulses are, as regards quality, always ♦ Schenck, "Archiv f. d. gesammte Physiologic," 118, L29, L907. PROPERTIES OF THE RETIXA. 357 the same, and differ from one another only in intensity, the qualitative differ- ences that exist among sensations being referred to a difference in reaction in the end-organ in the brain. Entoptic Phenomena. — Under the term entoptic phenomena is included a number of visual sensations due to the shadows of various objects within the eyeball itself. Ordinarily these shadows are imperceptible, owing to the diffuse illumination of the interior of the eye through the relatively wide opening of the pupil. By means of various devices the illumination of the eye may be so controlled as to make these shadows more distinct and thus bring the retinal images into consciousness. Some of these entopic ap- pearances are described briefly, but for a detailed description the reader is referred to the classical work of Helmholtz.* The Blood-corpuscles. — The entoptic images that are most easily recognized perhaps are those of the moving corpuscles in the capil- laries of the retina. If one looks off into the blue sky he will have no difficulty in recognizing a number of minute clear and dark specks that move in front of the eye in definite paths. The character of the movement leaves no doubt that these sensations are due to the shadows of the blood-corpuscles. In fact, the shadows often show a rhythmic acceleration in velocity synchronous with the heart- beats, a pulse movement. By projecting the moving images upon a screen at a known distance from the eye the velocity of the capil- lary circulation has been estimated in man. The Retinal Blood-vessels. — The blood-vessels of the retina lie in front of the rods and cones and must necessarily throw their shadows upon these sensitive end-organs. The shadows may be made more distinct and a visual picture of the vessels obtained by a number of methods. For instance, if a card with a pin hole through it is moved slowly in front of the eye the images of the blood-vessels stand out in the field of vision with more or less distinctness. The card should be given a circular movement. If it is kept in one position the images quickly disappear, since the retina apparently fatigues very quickly for such faint impressions. A more impressive picture may be obtained by the method of Purkinje. In a dark room one holds a candle toward the side of the head in such a position as to give the sensation of a glare in the corresponding eye. If the eye is directed toward the opposite side of the room and the candle is kept in continual circular movement the blood-vessels appear in the field of vision magni- fied in proportion to the distance of projection; the picture makes the impression of a thicket of interlacing branches. In this ex- periment the light from the candle strikes the nasal side of the ♦Helmholtz, "Handbuch der physiologischen Optik, " second edition, I, 184. 358 THE SPECIAL SENSES. retina at an oblique angle and is reflected toward the other side of the globe. The blood-vessels are in tins way illuminated from an unusual direction and their shadows are thrown upon a por- tion of the rethui not usually affected and for that reason perhaps more sensitive to the impression. Imperfections in the V itreous Humor and the Lens. — Small frag- ments of the cells from which the vitreous humor was constructed in the embryo and simi- lar relatively opaque ob- .a jects in the lens may A throw shadows on the |J retinal bottom. These shadows take different forms, but usually are de- scribed as small spheres or beads, single or in groups, that move with the eyes and are desig- nated, therefore, as the musca* volitantes (flitting flies or floating flies). To bring out these shadows it is convenient to make the source of illu- mination small and to bring it at or nearer than the anterior focal distance of the eye (15 to 16 mms.). The method employed for this purpose by Helmholtz is illustrated in Fig. 155. In this figure b is a candle flame, and a a lens of short focus which makes an image of the flame at the small opening shown in the dark screen, c. The eye is placed just behind this opening and is illuminated by the rays from the small, bright image of the flame at that spot. The shadows are seen projected upon the illuminated surface of the glass lens. Fig. 155. — Helmholtz's method of showing en- toptic phenomena due to imperfections in the lena and vitreous (Helmholtz): c, a screen with pinhole; a, lens with short focus. CHAPTER XIX. BINOCULAR VISION, Vision with two eyes differs from monocular vision chiefly in the varied combinations of movements of the two eyeballs and the aid thereby afforded in the determination of distance and size, in the enlarged field of vision, and, above all, in the more exact per- ception of solidity or perspective, especially for near objects. The Movements of the Eyeballs. — Each eyeball is moved by six extrinsic muscles which are innervated through three cranial nerves. The third or oculomotor nerve controls the internal rectus, the superior rectus, the inferior rectus, and the inferior oblique; the fourth cranial nerve (n. patheticus) innervates the superior oblique alone; and the sixth cranial (n. abducens) the external rectus alone. By means of these muscles the eyeballs may be given various movements, all of which may be considered as rotations of the ball around various axes. The common point of intersection of these axes is designated as the rotation point or center of rotation of the eyeball; it lies about 13.5 mms. back of the cornea in the emmetropic eye. The various axes of rotation all pass through this point, and we may classify them under four heads: (1) The horizontal or sagittal axis, which is the line passing through the rotation point and the object looked at, — the fixation point. This axis corresponds practically with the line of sight, — that is, the line drawn from the object looked at to the middle of the fovea, and it may therefore, without serious error, be spoken of as the visual axis. Rotations around this axis give a wheel movement or torsion to the eyeballs. (2) The transverse axis, the line passing through the rotation points of the two eyes and perpendicular to 1. Rotations around this axis move the eyeballs straight up or down. (3) The vertical axis, the vertical line passing through the rotation point and perpendicular at this point to the horizontal and transverse axes. Rotations around this axis move the eyeball to the right or the left. (4) The oblique axes, under which are in- cluded all the axes of rotation passing through the rotation point at oblique angles to the horizontal axis. These axes all lie in the equatorial plane of the eye, and rotations around any of them move the eyeball obliquely upward or 'downward. These definitions all have reference to what is known as the primary position of the 359 360 BINOCULAR VISION. eyes, — that is, that position taken by the eyes when we look straight before us toward the horizon, — a position, therefore, in which the plane of the horizontal axes is parallel to the ground; all other positions of the eyes are spoken of as secondary. With regard to the movements of the eyes about its axes of rotation the following general statements are made: Starting from the primary position, rotations of the eyes about the vertical axis — that is. movements directly to right or left — may be made by the contraction of the internal or the external rectus as the case may be. Rotations around the transverse axis — that is, movements directly up or down — require in each case the co-operation of two muscles. In movements upward the superior rectus, acting alone, would in Fig. l.">'i. " Diagram showing for the left eye the paths of the line of eight caused by the action of i in- different eye-muscles (Hering). The horizontal line indicates movements out or in to various degrees as caused by the contraction of the internal or external rectus. The Curved lines -how the amount of torsion Kiven the eyeball by tilt' superior and inferior Uperior and inferior oblique when contracting separately. The short line at the end of the paths indicates the position of the horizontal meridian at the end of the movement." R. <■., the external rectus; R, i., the internal rectus; /,'. S., ■ nor rectus; /.'. inf., the interior rectus; O. i., the inferior oblique; U. S., the superior oblique. rotating the eyeball upward also ^ive it a slight torsion so as to turn the upper pail of the vertical meridian inward. To obtain a movement directly upward (rotation around the transverse axis) tip- superior rectus and inferior oblique must act together. For a similar reason rotation directly downward requires the com- bined art ion of tip; inferior rectus and superior oblique. These facts are expressed dearly in Hering' s diagram, reproduced in Fig. 156, which indicates the paths traversed by the line of sight iball is moved by the different muscles acting sepa- rated. Rotation of the eyeballs around oblique axes require the co-operation of three of the muscles : movements upward and outward the Superior rectus, inferior oblique, and external rectus; movements upward and inward— superior rectus, inferior BINOCULAR VISION. 361 oblique, and internal rectus; movements downward and outward — inferior rectus, superior oblique, and external rectus; movements downward and inward — inferior rectus, superior oblique, and internal rectus. Most of the movements of the eyes are of the latter kind, — namely, rotations around an oblique axis, — and the position of the axis for each definite movement of this character may be determined by Listing's law, which may be stated as follows : When the eye passes from a primary to a secondary position it may be considered as having rotated around an axis perpendicular to the lines of sight in the two positions. It will be noted readily from observations upon the movements of one's own eyes that they ordinarily make only such movements as will keep the lines of sight of the two eyes parallel or will converge them upon a common point. In movements of convergence the internal recti of the two eyes are associated, while in symmetrical lateral movements the internal rectus of one eye acts with the external rectus of the other. Under normal conditions it is impossible for us to diverge the visual axes, — that is, to associate the action of the external recti. A movement of this kind would produce useless double vision (diplopia), and it is, therefore, a kind of movement which all of our experience has trained us to avoid. The Co-ordination of the Eye Muscles — Muscular Insuf- ficiency— Strabismus. — In order that the eyeballs may move with the minute accuracy necessary in binocular vision, a beautifully balanced or co-ordinated action of the opposing muscles is neces- sary. The object of these movements is to bring the point looked at in the fovea of each eye and thus prevent double vision, diplopia (see following paragraphs). This object is attained when the eye- balls are so moved that the lines of sight unite upon the object or point looked at. In viewing an object or in reading we keep readjusting the eyes continually to bring point after point at the junction of the lines of sight. When we look before us at a distant object the muscles in each eye should be so adjusted that without any contraction the antagonistic muscles will just balance each other — that is, when the eye muscles are entirely relaxed, except for their normal tone, the visual axes should be parallel. If this balance does not exist, we have a condition designated as heterophoria. In this condition a constant contraction of one or more muscles is required, even in far .vision, to prevent diplopia. When the eye at rest shows a tendency to drift toward the temporal side, owing to the fact that the pull of the external rectus overbalances that of the internal rectus, the condition is known as exophoria. If, for the opposite reason, there is a tendency to drift to the nasal side, the condition is described as esophoria. A tendency to drift up or down is called hyperphoria, and this is further specified as right 362 THE SPECIAL SENSES. or left hyperphoria according to the eye whose axis deviates upward. A lack of resting balance of this kind will make itself felt also in near work, particularly in reading, sewing, etc., since it will require a constantly greater innervation of the muscle whose antagonist overbalances it. Under some conditions the resulting muscular strain causes much uneasiness or distress. The heterophorias are easily detected and measured by the use of prisms, but they do not show the same constancy as the refractive errors of the eye, owing probably to the fact that they involve the variable factor of muscular tonus. The defect may be remedied by surgical operations upon the muscles or by the use of proper prisms with their bases so adjusted as to help the weaker muscle. In exophoria, for example, the greater pull of the external rectus rotates the front of the eye outward, while the back of the eye with the fovea is moved inward toward the nose. A prism of the proper strength placed before the eye with its base in toward the nose will throw the image of an external object on the fovea where it is, without necessitating a contraction of the internal rectus to bring the fovea back into its normal position. When the lack of balance between the opposing muscles is so great that the visual axes cannot by muscular effort be brought to bear upon the same points, we have the condition of squint or strabismus. Such a condition may result from a deficiency in strength or in actual paralysis of one or more of the muscles, or from an overaction in some of the muscles as contrasted with their antagonists. The Binocular Field of Vision. — When the two eyes are fixed upon a given point, placed, lei us say, in front of us in the median plane, each eye has its own visual field that may be charted by means of the perimeter. Hut the two fields overlap for a portion of their extent, and this overlapping; area constitutes the field of binocular vision (see big. 157). livery point in the bin- ocular field forms an image upon the two retinas. The most interesting fact about the binocular field is that some of the objects contained in it are seen single in spite of the fact that there are two retinal images, while others are seen or may he -een double when one'.- attention is directed to the fact. Whether any given object ii 9ingle or double depends upon whet her it- image does or does not fall upon corresponding points in the two retinas. Corresponding or Identical Points. — By definition corre- sponding or identical point- in t he I we retinas are t hose which when simultaneously stimulated by the same luminous object give us a single sensation, while non-corresponding points are those which when so Stimulated give us two visual sensations. It is evident, from our experience, that the fovea' form corresponding points or When ue look at any object we so move onr eyes that the BINOCULAR VISION. 363 image of the point observed shall fall upon symmetrical parts of the fovea ; the lines of sight of the two eyes converge upon and meet in the point looked at. If while observing an object we press gently upon one eyeball with the end of the finger, two images are seen at once, and they diverge farther and farther from each other as the pressure upon the eyeball is increased. Experiment shows, also, that, in a general way, portions of the retina symmetrically placed to the right side of the foveas in the two eyes are cor- responding, and the same is true for the two left halves and the two upper and lower halves. The right half of the retina in one eye is non-corresponding to the left half of the other retina, and vice * 061 081 OU Fig. 157. — Perimeter chart to show the extent of the binocular visual field (shaded area) when the eyes are fixed upon a median point in the horizontal plane. versa; and the same relation is true of the upper and lower halves, respectively. If we imagine one retina to be lifted without turning and laid over the other so that the foveas and vertical and horizontal meridians coincide, then the corresponding points will be superposed throughout those portions of the retina that represent the binocular field. This statement, however, is theoretical only; an exact point to point correspondence has not been determined experimentally. Experiments have shown, however, that the corresponding points in the upper halves of the retinas along the vertical mid-line do not cover each other, that is, they do not lie in the actual anatom- ical vertical meridian, but form two meridians which diverse 364 THE SPECIAL SENSES. symmetrically from the mid-line so as to make an angle of about 2 degrees (physiological incongruence of the retinas). Within the limits of our powers of observation for ordinary objects we may adopt Tscherning's rule, — namely, that when the images of an object on the two retinas are projected to the same side of the point of fixation they are seen single, their retinal images in this case falling on the retina to the same side of the lines of sight ; when, however, the retinal images fall on opposite sides of the lines of sight and are projected to opposite sides of the point of fixation, they are seen double. The doubling of objects that do not fall on corresponding points (physiological diplopia) is most readily demonstrated for objects that lie between the lines of sight, either closer or farther away than the object looked at. If, for instance, one holds the two forefingers in front of the face, in the median plane, one hand being at about the near point of distinct vision and the other as far away as possible, it will be noticed that when the eyes are fixed on the far finger the near one is seen double and vice versa. In this, as in other experiments in which the eyes are accommodated for one object while the attention is directed to another, some difficulty may be experienced at first in disso- ciating these two acts which normally go together, but a little practice will soon enable one to distinguish clearly the doubling of the point upon which the lines of sight are not converged. If a long stick is held horizontally in front of the eyes the end near the face will be doubled when the eyes are directed to the far end and vice versa. Moreover, by a simple experiment it may be shown that objects nearer the eyes than the point looked at are doubled heteronymously, — that is, the right-hand image be- longs to the left eye and the left-hand one to the right eye. This is easily demonstrated by closing the eyes alternately and noting which of the images disappears. The reason for the cross-projec- tion of the images is made apparent by the const miction in Fig. L58, /. bearing in mind the essential fact that in projecting our retinal images we always project to the plane of the object upon which the are focused. In the figure the eyes are converged on A ; the Images of point B fall to opposite sides of the line of sight and are See® double and are projected to the plane Of A, the image on the right eye being projected to //on the left of .1 and that on thelefteye to 6 on the right of A. In a similar way it may be shown that ob- farther away from t he eye than the point looked a1 are doubled homonymous!)',- that is, the right-hand image belongs to the right eye, and the left-hand one to the left eye. The fad is explained by the cob bruction in Fig. L58, II, in which A is the point converged upon and B the more distanl object. In all binocular vision, there- fore, the eriee of objects between the eye and thepoini looked at are BINOCULAR VISION. 365 doubled heteronymously, and those extending beyond the point in the same line are doubled homonymously. Normally we take no conscious notice of this fact, our attention being absorbed by the object upon which the lines of sight are directed. Some physi- ologists, however, have assumed that the knowledge plays an im- portant part subconsciously in giving us an idea of depth or per- spective,— an immediate perception, as it were, of the distinction between foreground and background. It is usually assumed that the explanation of corresponding points is to be found in the anatomical arrangement of the optic nerve fibers. Those from the right halves of the two retinas, which are corresponding halves, unite in the Fig. 158. — Diagrams to show homonymous and heteronymous diplopia: In 7 the eyes are focused on A; the images of B fall on non-corresponding points, — that is, to different sides of the fovea?, — and are seen double, being projected to the plane of A, giving heter- onymous diplopia. In // the eyes are focused on the nearer point, A, and the farther point, B, forms images on non-corresponding points and is seen double, — homonymous diplopia, — the images being projected to the focal plane A. right optic tract and are distributed to the right side of the brain, while the fibers from the left halves go to the left side of the brain. The basis of the single sensation from two visual images is to be found probably in the fact that the cerebral terminations through which the final psychical act is mediated lie close together or possibly unite. The Horopter. — In every fixed position of the eyes there are a certain number of points in the binocular field which fall upon corresponding points in the two retinas and are therefore seen single. The sum of these points is designated as the horopter for that position of the eyes. It may be a straight or curved line, or a plane or curved surface. Helmholtz calls attention to the fact that, when standing with our eyes in the primary position, — that is, directed toward the horizon, — the horopter is a plane coinciding with the ground, and this fact may possibly be of service to us in walking. Suppression of Visual Images. — It happens not infrequently that when an image of an object falls upon non-corresponding 366 THE SPECIAL SENSES. points in the two retinas the mind ignores or suppresses one of the images. This peculiarity is exhibited especially in the case of per- sons suffering from " squint " (strabismus). In this condition the individual, for one reason or another, is unable to adjust the contrac- tions of his eye muscles so as to unite his lines of sight upon the object looked at. The image of the object falls upon non-corre- sponding points and should give double vision, diplopia. This would undoubtedly be the case if the condition came on suddenly ; just as double vision results when we dislocate one eyeball by pressing slightly upon it. But in cases of long standing one of the images, that from the abnormal eye, is usually suppressed. The act of suppression seems to be a case of a stronger stimulus prevail- ing over a weaker one in consciousness, just as a painful sensation from stimulation of one part of the skin may be suppressed by a stronger pain from some other region. Struggle of the Visual Fields. — When the images of two dis- similar objects are thrown, one on each retina, the mind is presented, so to speak, simultaneously with two different sensations. Under such circumstances what is known as the struggle of the visual fields ensues. If the image on one eye consists of vertical lines and on the other of horizontal lines we see only one field at a time, first one then the other, or the field is broken, vertical lines in part and horizontal lines in part; there is no genuine fusion into a con- tinuous, constant picture. The struggle of the two fields is better illustrated when different colors are thrown on the two retinas. When red and yellow are superposed on one retina we obtain a com- pound sensation of orange; if they are thrown one on one retina, one on the other, no such fusion takes place. We see the field alternately red or yellow or a mixture of part red and part yellow, or at times one color, as it were, through the other. If, however, one field is white and the other black a peculiar sensation of glitter i- obtained, quite unlike the uniform gray that would result if the two fields were superposed on one retina. Judgments of Solidity. Our vision gives us knowledge not only of t he surface area of objects, but also of their depth or solidity, — that is, from our visual sensations we obtain conceptions of the three dimensions of space. The visual sensations upon which this conception i- buill are of several differenl kinds, partly monocular, — that is, such as are perceived by one eye .alone, partly binocular. If we close one eye and look a1 a bit of landscape or a solid object, ue are conscious of the perspective, of the right relations of fore- ground and background, and those individuals who have the misfortune to lose one eye are still capable, under most, circum- stances, of corred visual judgments concerning three dimen- sional mace. Nevertheless it i true that with binocular vision BINOCULAR VISION. 367 our judgments of perspective are more perfect, and that under certain circumstances data are obtained from vision with two eyes which give us an idea of solidity far more real than can be obtained with one eye alone. This difference is shown especially in the combination of stereoscopic pictures, and in ordinary vision when the light is dim, as in twilight, or in exact judgments of perspective in the case of objects close at hand. If, for example, we close one eye and attempt to thread a needle, light a pipe, or make any similar co-ordinated movement that depends upon an exact judgment of the distance of the object away from us, it will be found that the resulting movement is far less perfectly performed than when two eyes are used. The sensation elements upon which our judgments of depth or perspective are founded may be classified as follows:* The Monocular Elements. — That is, those that are experienced in vision with one eye. (a) Aerial perspective. The air is not en- tirely transparent, and, therefore, in viewing landscapes the more distant objects are less distinctly seen, as is illustrated, for instance, by the haze covering distant mountains. This experience leads us sometimes to make erroneous judgments when the conditions are unusual. An object seen suddenly in a fog looms large, as the expression goes, since the feeling that hazy objects are at a great distance leads us to give a proportional overvaluation to the rela- tively large visual image made by the near object. (b) Mathematical perspective. The outlines of objects before us are projected upon the surface of the eye in two dimensions only, just as they are represented in a drawing. The lines that indicate depth are therefore foreshortened, and lines really parallel tend to converge more and more to a vanishing point in proportion to their distance away from us. When one stands between the tracks of a railway, for instance, this convergence of the parallel lines is dis- tinctly apparent. We have learned to interpret this mathematical perspective correctly and with great accuracy. The use of this perspective in drawings is, in fact, one of the chief means employed by the artist to produce an impression of depth or solidity. For distant objects at least this factor is probably the most potent of those that can be appreciated by monocular vision. The importance of the mathematical perspective for our visual judgments may be illustrated very strikingly by a simple experiment. If one takes a biconvex lens of short focus and standing at a window that looks out upon a long street holds the lens in front of the eyes at arm's length he will be able to see, by focusing on the inverted image formed by the lens, that not only are objects inverted as regards their surface features, but, for most persons at least, the perspective is also inverted. Objects actually in the foreground will appear in the background, and one may have the curious sensations of watching persons who. as they walk, seem to recede farther and farther into * See Le Conte, " Sight," vol. 31 of "The International Scientific Series," 1881. 368 THE SPECIAL SENSES. the distance in spite of the fact that they continue to increase in size. The inverted or pseudoseopic vision thus produced is due undoubtedly to the in- version of the lines of perspective. Parallel lines which, without the lens, would have on the retina a project ion of t his kind /\ are with the lens projected inverted \/ , and our visual judgments arc controlled by this factor in spite of the opposing evidence from the size of the retinal images. In order lor the experiment to succeed it is necessary that the objects viewed shall be far enough away so that a flat picture may be given by the lens, — that- is, a pic- ture in which the foci for the near points shall not differ practically from those of more distant points, otherwise the muscular movements of accommodation interfere with the delusion. The relative importance of this last factor (see succeeding paragraph) is well illustrated by varying the experiment in this way: Place two objects upon a well-lighted table, one at the near end and one at the far end. Then standing close to the table view these objects through the lens as before. They will be seen in their right relations to each other. If, however, one backs away from the table while watching the images there will come a distance at which the near object will be seen to shift around to the rear of the far object. (c) The Muscle Sense (Focal Adjustment). — For objects near enough to require accommodation it is obvious that the nearer object will need a stronger contraction of the ciliary muscle, and also of the internal rectus in order to bring the line of sight to bear correctly. By means of the fibers of muscle sense we have a verv exact conception of the degree of contraction of these muscles, and this sensation is perhaps the most important factor used in making our monocular judgments of depth for objects at a short distance. In binocular vision the same factor is doubtless of increased effi- ciency by reason of the sensations obtained from the two eyes. (d) The disposition of lights ami shades and the size of familiar objects. It may be assumed that in distant vision of complex fields the varying lights and shades exhibited by objects according as they stand in front of or behind each other also aid our judg- ment. The actualsize alsoof the retinal images of familiar objects — such as animals, trees, etc. — gives us an accessory fact which con- tributes to the impression derived from the sources mentioned above. These factors are employed with effect by the artist in strengthening the general impression which he wishes to give of the difference between the foreground and the background. The Binocular Perspective. — In binocular vision there is an additional element which contributes greatly to our judgment of depth. Thifi element, consists in the fact that the retinal images of externa] objects, particularly near objects, are different in the two [nasmuch as the eyes are separated by some distance the projection of any .solid object upon one retina is different from the projection on the other. If a truncated pyramid is held in front of the eyes, the right eye sees more of the right side, the left more of the left side. The projection of the same object upon the two retina- may, in fact, be represented by the drawings given in Fig. J.'/.i. Whenever this condition prevails, whenever what we BINOCULAR VISION. 369 Fig. 159. — Right- and left-eyed images of truncated pyramid. May be combined to produce solid image by relaxing the accommodation, — that is, gazing to a dis- tance through the book. may call a right-eyed image of an object is thrown on the right eye and simultaneously a left-eyed image on the left eye, whether in nature or by an artifice, we at once perceive depth or solidity in the object. This fact is made use of in all devices employed to produce stereoscopic vision. Stereoscopic Vision. — Stereoscopic pictures may be obtained by photographing the same object or collec- tion of objects from slightly different points so as to get a right-eyed and a left- eyed picture ; or for simple outline pic- tures, such as geo- metrical figures, they may be made by draw- ings of the object as seen by the two eyes, respectively (see Figs. 159 and 161). Any optical device that will enable us to throw the right- eyed picture on the right eye and the left-eyed picture on the left eye constitutes a stereoscope. Many different forms of stereoscope have been devised; the one that is most frequently A ^ > used is the Brewster stereoscope :\ \ / represented in principle in Fig. 160. Each eye views its corre- sponding picture through a curved prism. The sight of the left-eyed picture is cut off from the right eye, and vice versa, by a partition extending for some distance in the median plane. The prisms are placed with their bases outward and the rays of light from the pictures are re- fracted, as shown in the diagram, so as to aid the eyes in converg- ing their lines of sight upon the same object. The prisms also magnify the pictures somewhat. Stereoscopic pictures are mounted usually for this instrument so that the distance between the same )>'£ Fig. 160. — Diagram to illustrate the principle of the Brewster stereoscope {Landois) : P and P', the prisms, a, 6, and a, 0, the left- and ri^ht-eyed pictures, respectively, b, P, being a point in the foreground and a, a, a point in the back- ground. The eyes are converged and focused separately for each point as in viewing naturally an object of three di- mensions. object in the two pictures is about 80 mms. — greater, therefore, than the interocular distance. A simple form of stereoscope that is very 24 170 THE SPECIAL SENSES. effective and interesting is sold under the name of the anaglyph. The two pictures in this case are approximately superposed, but the outlines of one are in blue and the other in red. When looked at, therefore, the picture gives an ordinary flat view with confused red-blue outlines. If, however, one holds a piece of red glass in front of the left eye and apiece of blue glass in front of the right eye, or more conveniently uses the pair of spectacles provided which have blue glass on one side, red on the other, then the picture stands out at once in solid relief with surprising distinctness — and as a black and white object only. The red and blue glasses in this case simply serve to throw the right-eyed image on the right eye and the left-eyed image on the left eye. Assuming that the right-eyed image is outlined in red, then the blue glass should be in front of the right eye. This glass will absorb the red rays completely so that the red outlines in the picture will seem black and a distinct right- eyed picture is thrown on the right eye, distinct enough to make us overlook the much fainter image in blue, which is also trans- mitted through the blue glass. The red glass before the left eye cuts out, in the same way, the right-eyed image and presents in dark outline the left-eyed image. By simply reversing the spectacles the right-eyed image may be thrown upon the left eye and vice versa. Under these conditions the picture for most per- sons may be seen in inverted relief (pseudoscopic vision), objects in the foreground re- ceding into the background. This inversion of the relief when the projection upon the retinas is reversed is a strik- ing indication of the potency of the normal projection as a factor in our judgments of solid objects. It will be ob- served, moreover, that those pictures that show least mathematical perspective are the most readily inverted, and that the ability to invert the picture varies in different individuals; in some, what we have called the binocular perspective, founded upon tlic dissimilar images, prevails over fhe mathematical perspective more readily than in others. Stereoscopic pictures may also be combined very successfully withoul i he He of :i stereoscope by virtue of the phenomenon of physiological diplopia. If, for instance, two stereoscopic drawings, such as are represented in Fig. 161, are held before the eyes and one relaxee hi accommodation bo as to look through the pictures, us it Fig. 161. — Stereosropie picture of an octahe- dral crystal. May be combined stereoscopically by relaxing the accommodation by the method of heteronymous diplopia. Hold the object at a distance of a fool or more and gaze beyond. BINOCULAR VISION. 371 were, to a point beyond, then, in accordance with what was stated on p. 364, each picture gives a double image, since it falls on non-corresponding parts of the two retinas. Four pictures, there- fore will be seen, all out of focus. With a little practice one can so converge his eyes as to make the two middle images come together, and since one of these is an image of the right-eyed picture and is falling on the right eye, and the other is a left-eyed picture falling on the left eye, the combination of the two fulfills the necessary conditions for binocular perspective. The figure stands out in bold relief. Explanation of Binocular Perspective. — Our perception of solidity or relief is a secondary psychical act, and, so far as the binoc- ular element is concerned, it is based upon the fact that the images are slightly different on the two retinas; but why this dissimilarity should produce an inference of this kind is not entirely understood. Certain facts have been pointed out as having a probable bearing upon the mental process. In the first place, in stereoscopic pictures, as in nature, we do not see the whole field at once. To see the ob- jects in the foreground the eyeballs must be converged by the eye muscles so that the lines of sight may meet in the object regarded. When attention is paid to objects in the background less convergence is necessary (see Fig. 158). The point of fixation for the lines of sight is kept continually moving to and fro, and the sensation of this muscular movement undoubtedly plays an important part in giving us the idea of depth or solidity. For persons not practised in the matter of observing stereoscopic pictures the full idea of relief comes out only after this muscular activity has been called upon. But for the practised eye this play of the muscles is not absolutely necessary. The stereoscopic picture stands out in relief even when illuminated momentarily by the light of an electric spark. The per- ception of solidity in this case is instantaneous, and it has been sug- gested that this result may depend upon the immediate recognition of physiological diplopia, — that is, the fact that objects nearer than the point of fixation are doubled heteronymously, while those farther away are doubled homonymously (see p. 364). Such an effect can only be produced distinctly by objects having depth and possibly in the case of the trained eye it alone is sufficient to give the immediate inference of solidity or relief, while the un- trained eye requires the accessory sensations aroused by focal adjustment, mathematical perspective, etc. Judgments of Distance and Size. — Judgments of distance and size are closely related. Our judgments regarding size are based primarily upon the size of the retinal image, the amount of the visual angle. This datum, however, is sufficient in itself only for objects at the same distance from us. If they are at different 372 THE SPECIAL SENSES. distances or we suppose that such is the case, our judgment of the distance controls our judgment of size. This fact is beautifully shown in the case of after-images (see p. 343). When an after- image of any object is obtained on the retina our judgment of its size depends altogether on the distance to which we project it. If we look at a surface near at hand, it seems small; if we gaze at a wall many feet away it is at once greatly enlarged. The familiar instance of the variation in the size of the full moon according as it is seen at the horizon or at the zenith depends upon the same fact. The distance to the horizon as viewed along the surface of the earth seems greater than to the zenith; we picture the heavens above us as an arched dome flattened at the top, and hence the same size of retinal image is interpreted as larger when we suppose that we see it at a greater distance. Our judgments of distance, on the other hand, depend primarily upon the data already enumerated in speaking of the perception of solidity or depth in the visual field. For objects within the limit of accommodation we depend chiefly on the muscle sense aroused by the act of focusing the eyes, — that is, the contractions of the ciliary and of the extrinsic muscles. For objects outside the limit of accommodation we are influenced by binocular perspective, mathematical perspective, aerial perspective, -A _ B Fig. 162. — Muller-Lyer figures to show illusion in space perception. The lines A and B are of the same length. etc. But here again our judgment of distance is greatly influenced in the case of familiar objects by the size of the retinal image. A .striking instance of the latter fact is obtained by the use of field glasses or opera glasses. When we look through them properly the size of the retinal image is enlarged, and the objects, therefore, seem to be nearer to US. If we reverse the glasses and look through the large end the size of the retinal image is reduced and the objects, 1 herefore, seem to be much farther away, since under normal condi- tions such small images of familiar objects are formed only when they are a1 a great distance from us. Optical Deceptions. Wrong judgments as regards distance and size are frequently made and the fad may be illustrated id a number of interesting ways. Thus, in Fig. L62 the lines .1 and B are of the same length, but B seems to be distinctly the longer. So in Fig, 103 the vertical lines, although exactly parallel, seem, on the c<> illu-t rate binocular deceptions depending upon different inter- pretation of the mathematical perspective and the lights and shades. On gazing fixedly tin- image will Change From B single cube with black top resting on two others with black top , to one ')! two cube with black bottoms resting upon a single cube with black bottom. .Still other figures may appear from time to time. focuses for the point a, this point may be held in the foreground and the second of the above appearances be scon. While if the are accommodated Btrongly for point &, it will be brought forward and the firsl of the two appearances described is brought into view. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE EAR. CHAPTER XX. THE EAR AS AN ORGAN FOR SOUND SENSATIONS. In discussing the physiology of the ear it is necessary to consider the functional importance of its various parts, the external ear consisting of the lobe or pinna, the external auditory meatus, and the tympanic membrane; the middle ear, with its chain of ossicles, its muscles and ligaments, and the Eustachian tube; and the internal ear, with its cochlea, vestibule (utriculus and sacculus), and semi- circular canals. The eighth cranial or so-called auditory nerve is Fig. 166. — Semidiagrammatic section through the right ear (Czermak): G, External auditory meatus; T, membrana tympani; P, tympanic cavity; o, fenestra ovalis; r, fen- estra rotunda; B, semicircular canal; aS, cochlea; Vt, scala vestibuli; Pt, scala tympani; E, Eustachian tube. distributed entirely within the internal ear; the fibers of the coch- lear branch, which alone perhaps are concerned with hearing, end among the sensory nerve cells of the cochlea, while the vestibular branch supplies similar sense cells situated in the utriculus, sacculus, and the ampullse of the semicircular canals. We may consider first the functions of the ear in respect to the sensations of sound. 375 376 THE SPECIAL SENSES. The somewhat complicated anatomy of the parts concerned should be obtained from the special works on anatomy or histology. For the purposes of a physiological presentation the scheznatic figure employed by Czermak and reproduced in Fig. 166 will suffice to exhibit the general anatomical relations of the parts concerned in the transmission of the sound waves from the exterior to the cochlea. The Pinna or Auricle. — The pinna opens into the external mea- tus by means of a cone-shaped depression, the concha. The whole organ, and especially the concha, may be considered as fulfilling more or less perfectly the function of collecting the sound waves and reflecting them into the meatus. In the lower animals the con- cave shape of the ear and its motility probably make it much more useful in this respect than in the case of the human ear. But even in man the pinna is valuable to some extent in intensifying the appreciation of sounds and also in enabling us to deter- mine their direction. The ex- ternal auditory meatus has a length of about 21 to 26 mms., and a capacity of something over one cubic centimeter. Its course is not straight, but passes first somewhat back- ward and upward, and then turns forward and inward to end against the tympanic membrane. All sound waves that affect the drum of the ear must, of course, pass through this canal. The Tympanic Mem- brane.— The tympanic mem- brane closes the inner end of the meatus and lies obliquely to the axis of the canal, its plane rnuking an angle, opening downward, of 150 degrees. The mem- brane, although not more than 0.1 mm. thick, consists of three coats: a layer of skin on the external surface, a layer of mucous membrane on the side; toward the middle ear, and in between a layer of fibrous connective tissue. The middle layer gives to the membrane its peculiar structure and properties. In form the mini! 'nine h:is the shape of a shallow funnel with the apex, or umbo, OS it Is called, somewhat below the center. The fibers of the fibrous layer ure arranged partly circularly and partly in lines radiating from the umbo to the peripheral margin (Fig. 167). The walls of the funnel are slightly convex outwardly; so that -To show the structure of the tympanic membrane, looked at from the side of the meatus (f/ensen): ax, The axis of rota- tion of the ear bones; d, the incus; a, the head of the malleus. EAR AS AN ORGAN FOR SOUND SENSATIONS. 377 each radiating fiber forms an arch. On the inner side of the mem- brane the chain of ear ossicles is attached, so that the vibrations of the membrane are transmitted directly to these bones. The peculiar form of the membrane, its funnel shape, its arched sides, and its unsymmetrical division by the umbo are supposed to con- tribute to its value as a transmitter of the sound vibrations of the air. In the first place, the membrane shows little tendency to after-vibrations, — that is, when set in motion by an air wave it shows little or no tendency to continue vibrating after the acting Fig. 168. — Tympanum of right side with ossicles in place, viewed from within (after Morris): 1, Body of incus; 2, suspensory ligament of malleus; 3, ligament of incus; 4, head of malleus; 5, epitympanic cavity; 6, chorda tympani nerve; 7, tendon of tensor tympani muscle; 8, foot-piece of stirrup; 9, os orbiculare; 10, manubrium; 11, tensor tympani muscle; 12, membrana tympani; 13, Eustachian tube. force has ceased. It is obvious that such a property is valuable in rendering hearing more distinct, and the peculiarity of the mem- brane in this respect is attributed partly to its special form and partly to the damping action of the bones attached to it. In the second place, the arched sides of the funnel act as a lever, so that the movements at these parts are transmitted to the umbo with a diminution in amplitude, but an intensification in force. It is at the umbo that the movement is communicated to the ear bones. The Ear Bones. — The three ear bones — the malleus, the incus, and the stapes — taken together form a chain connecting the tym- ! 78 THE SPECIAL SENSES. panic membrane with the membrane of the fenestra ovalis. By this means the vibrations of the tympanic membrane are com- municated to the membrane of the fenestra ovalis and thus to the perilvmph filling the cavity of the internal ear. The bones consist of spongy material with a compact surface layer. Their general shape and connections are illustrated in Figs. 168 and 169. To understand the manner in winch the chain of bones acts in con- veying the vibrations from one membrane to the other some points in their structure and connections may be recalled. The malleus is about IS to 19 mms. long, and has an average weight of 23 milli- grams. Its long handle is imbedded in the tympanic membrane, the tip reaching to the umbo. The large, rounded head projects above the upper edge of the tympanic membrane and forms a true joint of a peculiar nature with the incus. It has two processes in ad- dition to the manubrium : a short one, processus brevis, that presses against the upper edge of the tym- panic membrane, and a longer one, the processus gracilis or processus Folianus, which projects forward and is continued bj^ a ligament, the anterior ligament, through which the malleus is attached to the bony wall of the tympanic cavity. Three other ligaments are attached to the malleus, the external ligament, bind- ing it to the external face of the cavity, the posterior ligament, and the superior ligament, the latter at- taching the upper part of the head to the roof of the tympanic cavity. By means of these ligaments the bone is held steadily in position even after its connections with the incus are loosened. The incus is somewhat more mas- sive than the malleus, weighing about 25 milligrams. Its thicker portion articulates with the head of the malleus, and it has two proff—cs nearly at right angles to each other. The shorter process extends posteriori;, and is attached by a ligament to the posterior wall of the tympanic cavity; the long process passes downward parallel with the handle of the malleus, but turns in at the tip to form the rounded os orbiculare, which articulates with the of the stapes. This latter Lone is extremely light, weighing about 3 milligrams, its oval base heing attached to the margins of the fenestra ovalis by s Bhort, stiff membrane. The Mode of Action of the Ear Bones. — The movements of Fig. 169.— The bones of the middle ear in natural connections (Helmholtz): M, The malleus; Mcp, the head; Mc, the neck; Ml, the Erocessus gracilis; Mm, the rnanu- rium; Ic, body of the incus; lb, short proces.-); //, long process; ', ('/', Fig. 171), whose lower portion does not reach to the basilar membrane, but is supported by the intervening Deiters cells. The upper ends of the cells project, through the openings in the reticulate membrane and end in a number— according to Retains,* about twenty — short, stiff hairs. The hair cells are arranged in four to six rows, one • The moel complete details of the structure of the ear will be found in the great work of Etetzius, "Das GehOrorgan der Wirbelthiere," vol. ii, 1884, Stockholm. EAR AS AN ORGAN FOR SOUND SENSATIONS. 383 row on the inner side of the inner rods of Corti and three to five rows, according to the part of the cochlea examined, on the outer side of the rods of Corti. Their total number has been estimated differently by different observers ; but, accepting the lower figures given, it may be said that there are at least 3500 inner hair cells and 13,000 outer ones, giving a total of 16,500 or more. The theory usually proposed to account for the mechanism by which the vibra- tions of the perilymph affect these cells, and especially the expla- nation of the means by which different sounds affect different cells, is that there is contained in the cochlea a mechanism which acts by sympathetic resonance. To make this theory clear a short Fig. 171. — Diagrammatic view of the organ of Corti, the sense cells, and the accessory structures of the membranous cochlea (Testut): A, Inner rods of Corti ; B, outer rods of Corti ; C, tunnel of Corti ; D, basilar membrane; E, single row of inner hair (sense) cells; 6, 6', 6", rows of outer hair (sense) cells; 7, 7', supporting cells of Deiters. The ends of the inner hair cells are seen projecting through the openings of the reticulate membrane. The terminal arborizations of the cochlear nerve fibers end around the inner and outer hair cells. description must be given of the nature of sound waves and the physical facts in regard to sympathetic resonance. The Nature and Action of Sound Waves. — Sound waves in air consist of longitudinal vibrations of the air molecules, alternate phases of rarefaction and condensation. For convenience' sake, these waves are usually represented graphically after the manner of water waves, by a curved line rising above and falling below a median zero line, the ordinates above the zero line representing the phase of condensation, and those below the phase of rarefaction. These waves are produced by the vibrations of the sounding bod}r, and may vary greatly in length, in amplitude, and in form. For 3S4 THE SPECIAL SENSES. musical sounds within the range of hearing the length of the waves may vary from forty to seventy feet, at the one extreme, to a frac- tion of an inch at the other. They travel through the air with an average velocity of 1100 to 1200 feet per second, the exact rate vary- ing with the temperature. When these waves, whatever may be their form, follow each other with regularity — that is, with a definite period or rhythm — a musical sound is perceived provided the rhythm is maintained for a number of vibrations. 80 that regular- ity or periodicity of the sound waves may be considered as the un- derlying physical cause of musical sounds. Non-musical sounds or noises, which constitute the vast majority of our auditory sensa- tions, are referred, on the contrary, to non-periodical vibrations. Waves of this kind may be due to the nature of the impulse given to the air by the sounding body, — single pulses, for instance, or a series of such pulses or shocks following at a slow or irregular rhythm, or as is more frequently the case, they may result from a mixture of very short and different rhythmical vibrations. As the case of musical sounds is far the simpler, the theory of the action of the cochlea has been based chiefly upon the results obtained from a study of these forms of waves. Classification and Properties of Musical Sounds. — Musical sounds exhibit three fundamental properties, each of which may be referred to a difference in the physical stimulus. They vary, in the first place, in pitch, and this difference finds its explanation in the rapidity of vibration of the sounding body and the sound waves produced by it. The more rapid the rate, the shorter will be the waves and the higher will be the pitch of the musical note. Notes of the same pitch may, however, vary in loudness or intensity, and this difference is referable to the amplitude of the vibrations. A given tuning fork emits always a note of the same pitch, but the loudness of the note may vary according to the amplitude of the vibrations. The vibrations of the tympanic membrane and of the perilymph in the internal ear vary in rate and intensity with the sounding body; so that we may say that the stimulation of the hair cells in the cochlea gives us auditory sensations that vary in pitch with the rate of excitation and in intensity with the ampli- tude of the vibratory movement. A third properly of musical sounds is tlicir variations in quality or timbre. The same note of one amplitude when given by different musical instruments varies in quality, so that we have no difficulty m recognizing the note of a piano from the same note when given by a violin or the human voice. The underlying physical cause of variationsin timbre is found in 1 he form of the sound waves produced, and immediately, therefore, in the form of vibratory movement communicated to the perilymph. Examination of 1 he forms of sound waves produced EAR AS AN ORGAN FOR SOUND SENSATIONS. 385 by different musical instruments shows that they may be divided into two great groups: (1) The simple or penclular form; (2) the compound or non-pendular form. The simple or pendular form of wave is given, for instance, by tuning forks. A graphic repre- sentation of this wave form may be obtained by attaching a bristle to the end of the fork and allowing it to write upon a piece of black- ened paper moving with uniform velocity, — the blackened surface, for instance, of a kymographion. The form of the wave obtained is represented in Fig. 172. The vibrating body swings symmetrically to each side of the line of rest, and, inasmuch as this is also the form of movement that would be traced by a swinging pendulum, this form of wave is designated frequently as pendular. It is sometimes called also the sinusoidal wave, since the distance of the vibrating point to each side of the line of rest is equal to the sine of an arc increasing proportionally for the time of the phase. A compound (or non-pendular or non-sinusoidal) wave may have a very great variety of forms. The different phases follow periodically, but the movement of the vibrating body to each side of the line of rest is not Fig. 172. — Form of wave made by tuning fork. perfectly symmetrical. Fourier has shown that any periodical vibra- tory movement, whatever may be its form, may be considered as being composed of a series of simple or pendular movements whose periods of vibrations are 1, 2, 3, 4, etc., times as great as the vibra- tion period of the given movement. That is, every so-called com- pound wave form may be considered as being caused by the fusion of a number of simple waves. Representing the wave movement of the air graphically as water waves, this composition of simple waves into compound ones is illustrated by the curves given in Fig. 173. In this figure A and B represent two simple vibrations such as would be given by two tuning-forks, the vibrations in B being double those of A. If these two waves are communicated to the air at the same time the actual movement of the molecules will be a resultant of the forces acting upon them at any given instant, and the actual movement will be indicated, therefore, by the algebraical sum of the ordinates above and below the lines of rest. If the movements are so timed that e in curve B is synchronous with d° in curve A, then the resulting compound wave form is illustrated by C. If, however, curve B is supposed to be in a different phase, so that e is synchronous with d' , then a form of wave illustrated by D will be 25 3S6 THE SPECIAL SENSES. obtained. In this way a great variety of forms of compound waves may be supposed to be produced by the union of a series of simple waves of different periods of vibration. That compound waves dif- fer from simple ones in being composed of several series of vibrations is indicated directly by our sensations. When we listen to the note of a tuning-fork we hear only a single tone; when two or more tuning-forks are sounded together the trained ear can detect the tone due to each fork, and similarly when a single note is sounded by the human voice, a violin, or any other instrument that has a char- acteristic quality the trained ear can detect a series of higher tones, Fig, 173.— Schema by Helmholtz to illustrate (lie formation of a compound wave from two [jcmliihtr waves: A and /.', pendular vibrations, B being die octave of A. If superpo ed o that e coincides with d" and the ordinates arc added algebraically, the non- pendular curve C is produced. If .superposed so that e coincides with i produced. the upper partial tones, or harmonics, or overtones, which indicate that the note is really compound, and not simple. The formation of these overtones is due to the fact that the sounding body vibrates ri'.t Mnl- :i a whole, but also in its aliquot parts, as may be repre- sented in Fig. 174, illustrating the vibrations of a string. When I lie string is plucked it vibrates as a whole (a), giving large waves which produce what is called the fundamental lone, but at the same time each half (6), third (c), fourth (<1), etc., may vibrate, giving each its own simple tone. The combination of all of these simple waves forms a Compound wave whose form or at least whose EAR AS AN ORGAN FOR SOUND SENSATIONS. 387 composition determines the quality of the tone heard. As many as ten or sixteen of these overtones ma}- be detected from the vibrating strings of a violin or guitar. When the period of vibra- tion of these overtones bears a simple ratio to that of the funda- mental, a ratio that can be expressed by the simple numbers, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, they harmonize with it and form the harmonic overtones. It should be borne in mind that, so far as the tympanic membrane is concerned, it does not respond separately to the single tones which constitute the compound wave, but swings in unison with the movement of the compound wave.' Nevertheless the internal Fig. 174. — To illustrate the mechanism of the formation of overtones. — (Helmholtz.) In a the string vibrates as a whole, giving its fundamental tone; in b, c, and d, its halves, thirds, and fourths are vibrating independently. When a string is struck, plucked, or bowed these movements may happen simultaneously and the fundamental note due to the vibra- tions of the whole string is combined with the notes due to the vibrations of aliquot parts, the overtones. The combination gives a compound wave whose form and musical quality vary with the number and relative strength of the overtones. ear, according to the law of Ohm, is capable of analyzing the com- pound wave form into the series of simple or pendular wave forms of which it is composed and of distinguishing the series of corre- sponding tones. While this analysis cannot be made consciously except by the trained musician, it is made unconsciously, as it were, by even- normal ear, and in consequence of this analysis we recognize the variations in quality of different compound tones. The principle upon which the cochlea acts in thus separating the compound tones into their elements is not explained with entire satisfaction. According to the view so admirably presented by >n THE SPECIAL SENSES. Helmholtz,* the analysis depends upon the existence in the ear of a mechanism for sympathetic vibrations or resonance. Sympathetic Vibrations or Resonance. — By sympathetic vibration is meant the fact that an elastic body is easily set into vibration by movements of the surrounding medium when these movements correspond with its own period of vibration. A string whose period of vibration is 12S per second will be little affected by vibrations of the surrounding air unless they have the same periodicity. If, however, a note of this period is sounded by the voice, for instance, the string will be set into vibration with rela- tive ease. By means of this principle the untrained ear can readily pick out the more prominent of the upper harmonics of any given note of a musical instrument. It is only necessary to select a series of resonators corresponding to the series of overtones. Each reso- nator is set into vibration by its corresponding overtone and so emphasizes this particular tone that it may be easily recognized. If one stands in front of a piano with the strings exposed and sings a given note it may be shown that a series of the piano strings is set into vibration corresponding, in the first place, to the rate of vibration of the fundamental tone, and secondly to the more prominent of the harmonic overtones. In this case the com- pound wave strikes upon the collection of strings of the piano, and is analyzed into its component simple tones by the sympa- thetic vibrations of the corresponding strings. Helmholtz assumes that the cochlea analyzes compound musical waves by an essentially similar method. The Functions of the Cochlea. — The vibratory movement, whatever may be its form, in the air of the external meatus im- parts to the tympanic membrane a similar form of movement, and this, in turn, through the ear bones and the membrane of the fenes- tra ovalis sets the perilymph into vibrations of the same form. That the perilymph can swing or vibrate under the influence of the move- ments of the stapes is explained by the existence of the second opening, the fenestr.-i rotunda, between the middle and the inter- nal car (see Fig. 166). As the membrane of the fenestra ovalis is pushed in. flint of the fenestra rotunda is pushed out, and vice These vibratory movements of the perilymph affect the membranous cochlea, which may be regarded as being suspended in the perilymph, and according to the resonance theory certain structures within the membranous cochlea are set into sympa- thetic- vibrations corresponding to the simple waves of which the compound wave is constituted. Helmholtz first suggested that the peculiar rods of Corti form the resonating apparatus and by sympathetic vibrations are capable of analyzing the compound * Helmholtz, loc cit. EAR AS AN ORGAN FOR SOUND SENSATIONS. 389 movement. Later, however, this suggestion was abandoned, since the number of the rods is not sufficiently great perhaps to answer the requirements of this theory. According to Retzius, the inner rods number 5600 and the outer ones 3850. Moreover, these structures are absent from the bird's cochlea, and we must assume that these animals are capable of appreciating musical sounds. Helmholtz then adopted a suggestion of Hensen's, that the basilar membrane constitutes the resonating apparatus. This membrane forms the floor of the membranous cochlea, stretching from the limbus to the opposite side of the bony cochlea (Fig. 171). Its middle layer consists of fibers, running radially, which, though united to one another, are sufficiently independent to be regarded as separate strings. These fibers in the portion covered by the rods of Corti, the inner zone or zona tecta, are finer and more difficult to separate than in the portion exterior to the outer rods, the outer zone or zona pectinata. From the base to the apex of the cochlea the membrane increases in width, the length of the strings in the outer zone varying, according to Retzius, from 135 tt in the basal portion to 220 li in the middle spiral and to 234 tt at the apex. The whole structure is estimated to contain about 24,000 strings varying gradually in length, as stated, and resembling in general arrangement the strings of the piano. Assuming that each of these fibers has its own period of vibration, we may imagine that the entire collection forms an apparatus for sympathetic vibration which is capable of analyzing each compound wave motion into its constituent simple waves, each string being set into strong- est vibrations by the wave of the corresponding period. More- over, it is implied or assumed in this theory that the vibrations of each string are communicated to a corresponding nerve fiber of the cochlear nerve, through which the stimulus is conveyed to the brain as a nerve impulse. We should be capable of perceiv- ing, theoretically, as many distinct musical tones as there are fibers in the basilar membrane, while a compound wave, by setting a number of these mechanisms into action, gives a series of sensations which are more or less fused in consciousness. The peculiar quality or timbre of the tone of each instrument is refer- able, therefore, immediately to the number and relative intensities of the simple tone sensations that it arouses. The fusion of these elementary tone sensations into compound ones of different qual- ities is comparable, in a general way, to the fusion of simple color sensations, with this exception, however, that in the compound tone sensations we are capable of distinguishing more clearly the fact that they are composed of simpler elements ; the constituent tones may be recognized by the trained ear at least. The mechan- ism by which the vibrations of the strings of the basilar mem- 390 THE SPECIAL SENSES. brane are conveyed to the hair cells and through them to the nerve fibers is a matter of speculation only, as are also the functions of the remaining parts of the organ of Corti. It may be suggested, perhaps, that the rods of Corti and Deiters's cells, together with the reticulate membrane, with which the}' are both connected, form not only a supporting apparatus for the hair cells, but also a mechanism by which the vibrations of the strings are commu- nicated to the hairs of the hair cells; but the suggestion is unsatis- factory, as the anatomical arrangement does not suffice to explain how the vibrations of individual strings are transmitted to the separate hair cells. The assumption has also been made that the tectorial membrane acts as a damper to the vibrating hair cells or the reticulate membrane. Its position as a pad lying over the rods of Corti and the reticulate membrane justifies perhaps such an assumption. Many physiologists, while accepting the general principle that the cochlea analyzes the sound waves by a mechan- ism for sympathetic vibrations, have been unwilling to admit that the basilar membrane constitutes such a mechanism. They point to the improbability or impossibility of fibers of only 0.36 mm. (or 0.5 mm. at the best) in length acting as efficient reso- nators, especially as they are not entirely free and are surrounded by liquid. Attempts have been made, therefore, to select other structures in the cochlea as more likely to be affected by sympa- thetic vibrations. Attention has been directed mainly to the tectorial membrane or membrane of Corti. Thus, Ayers* believes that this structure as seen in the usual microscopical prepara- tions, is simply an artefact. Under normal conditions he believes that it is a band of very long and delicate hairs projecting from the hair cells and lying free in the endolymph. According to his view, it is these hairs that take up the vibrations and transmit their impulses directly to the hair cells. The histological statement upon which this view is based has not, however, been verified. More recently v. Ebner,f reviving an older view of Basse, has suggested that the tectorial membrane, especially its U-vc end, serves as the mechanism for sympathetic vibration. This membrane increases in width from the base to the apex of the cochlea and varies in thickness in its radial diameter, so that it might be conceived to md to different periods of vibrations in its different pads, it- movements being communicated directly to the hair cells upon which it, rest Unfortunately we have no direct, experimental evidence in favor of any of these views. Several observers, how- ever, have demonstrated apparently that, whatever may be the « Avers, "Journal <>f Morphology." 6, 1, l.s!>'_'. * /were, journal <>i worpnoiogy. o, i, iv.u. t Kollil.i r, " Handbuch a. Gewebelehre," sixth edition, vol. Hi, pt. u, p. 968, 1902. EAR AS AN ORGAN FOR SOUND SENSATIONS. 391 mechanism for sympathetic vibration, it is so arranged that at the base of the cochlea the higher notes are received and at the apex the notes of the lowest pitch. Thus, Munk, in experiments upon dogs, in which by an operation through the fenestra rotunda he had destroyed the basal portion of the cochlea, found that the animals, after a temporary deafness of some days, could hear apparently only low tones and noises. Baginsky,* in a later series of experi- ments, opened the bulla ossea on each side, destroyed the cochlea on one side entirely so as to render that ear deaf, while on the other he injured it in certain areas only. He found that when the apex of the cochlea was des- troyed the animal appeared to perceive only the high tones, c'", c"", e'"". The fundamental principle of the theory of the function of the cochlea as developed by Helmholtz has been subjected to some criticism. The theory of a series of resonators each responding to a definite note does not explain with entire satisfaction some of the known acoustic phenomena. Thus, it is known that when two notes are sounded together combinational tones may be heard, either a low difference tone whose pitch is equal to that of the difference between the rates of the two notes, or a summation tone whose pitch is equal to the sum of the vibrations of the two notes. It is difficult to conceive that these combina- tional tones have an objective existence, as vibrations, and the means by which they are perceived by the cochlea is not explained satisfactorily by the theory of resonators. Other theories of the function of the cochlea have been proposed to avoid such difficulties. Thus, Ewald f suggests a view according to which the basilar membrane vibrates throughout its length for each note. He has shown that a rubber membrane of the dimensions of the basilar membrane will be set into such vibrations throughout its length and when examined under the microscope presents such a pic- ture as is represented in Fig. 175, in which the crests of the waves are at a fixed interval for each tone. If at these intervals the corresponding hair cells and nerve fibers are supposed to be stimulated, then our consciousness would recognize each note by its appro- priate interval. For the application of this theory to musical harmony — combinational tones and beats — reference must be made to the original. Sensations of Harmony and Discord. — The combination of notes to produce various harmonies or intentional discords is a part of the theory of music, but attention may be called briefly to the physiological explanation offered by Helmholtz to account for the fact that certain notes when combined give us a disagreeable sen- sation, appear rough and unpleasant; while others, on the contrary, produce pleasant sensations. Discord or dissonance is due, accord- * Baginsky, "Virchow's Archiv f. pathol. Anat." 94, 65, 1SS3. t Ewald. "Archiv f. d. gesammte Physiologie," 76, 147, 1S99. Fig. 175.— To il- lustrate the idea of a fixed sound wave. — (.Ewald.) The illus- tration shows a fun- damental note and its first overtone. 392 THE SPECIAL SENSES. ing to Helmholtz, to the beats produced when two dissonant notes are sounded together. On the physical side the beat, — that is, a rhythmical variation in the intensity of the sound, — is due to the phenomenon of interference. If the rates of vibration of two notes are such that at certain intervals the crests of the waves fall to- gether and again the crest of one coincides with the hollow of the other, the sound sensations will be periodically increased and decreased. While there is no fundamental explanation for the fact that a regularly varying intensity of sound is disagreeable, it is a well-known phenomenon and it finds analogies in the other sensations,— for instance, in the very disagreeable effect of a flick- ering light. When two notes are sounded together the number of beats varies with the difference between the rates of vibration; thus, two notes, one of 128 vibrations and the other of 136 vibra- tions, give 8 beats per second. When the number of beats rises to 33 per second the discord is most disagreeable ; if, however, the rate of interference is more rapid, the unpleasant sensation becomes less perceptible, and beyond 132 per second is not notice- able. When the rates of vibrations of two tones are such that neither the fundamentals nor any of the overtones give beats, the effect is that of harmony, the vibrations of one note strengthening those of the other. The most perfect harmony is that of a note sounded simultaneously with another of the same rate, ratio 1:1, or with its octave, ratio 1: 2. The various intervals which in mi isic have been found to be perfectly consonant or which vary so little from it as to be usable in harmonies are those whose vibra- tions bear a simple ratio to each other. Thus, the octave of any note has the ratio of 1:2, the double octave 1 : 4, the twelfth 1 : 3. These three intervals give absolutely consonant sounds. Other intervals — such as the fifth, 2:3, or the major third, 4: 5 — give ;i less perfect consonance. Three or more notes bearing such rela- tion- to each other constitute a chord, the vibrations in the major chord being, for instance, in the ratios 4:5:G, — c' (128), e' (160), :!<> per second, although some -•in -till respond to an octave lower, about Hi per second. To vibrations below ::<» per second are fell , if perceived al nil, ogle pulses thai stimulate the sensory nerves of the tympanic membrane itself. The cochlea does not respond. It may happen, EAR AS AN ORGAN FOR SOUND SENSATIONS. 393 however, that a vibration too slow to be perceived by the ear will give overtones of sufficient strength to be recognized. An inter- esting example in physiology of this fact is furnished by the tone of the contracting muscle. As heard, this tone corresponds to a vibration of 40 per second; but other data lead us to believe that the vibrations of the contracting muscle, due to the single contrac- tions of which the compound contraction is composed, occur at the rate of only 10 per second; so that what is heard is, on this theory, the second octave of the fundamental. The high limit of audibility, on the other hand, is usually placed at 40,000 double vibrations per second, although the various estimates published vary so widely that in this respect there must be great individual differences. The shrill notes of insects are said to be inaudible to some ears. Konig, making use of Kundt's method of light powders, succeeded in tuning a series of forks to an estimated rate of 90,000 double vibrations per second. It was found that those between c7 and c9 (8192 to 32,768) were generally audible, while the c10 (65,536) was inaudible. The limit, therefore, lay between c9 and c10. Notes near this high limit are not, however, usable in ordinary music; the sensations produced have a disagreeable, if not actually painful shrillness. The range of vibrations employed in music is illustrated by the seven octaves of the piano, the notes varying from the lowest c of 32 vibrations to c6 of 4096 vibrations. The intervening series is divided into tones whose serial relations to each other are ex- pressed by the ratios f or V and semitones of the ratio yf or ff ; ^hus, c"= 256 vibrations and the d" of the same octave corresponds :'See Helmholtz, Popular Scientific Lectures, "Ueberdie phvsiologischen Ursachen des musikalischen Harmonie," Bonn, 1857. CHAPTER XXI. THE FUNCTIONS OF THE SEMICIRCULAR CANALS AND THE VESTIBULE. Position and Structure. — The membranous semicircular canals lie within the bony semicircular canals, the space between being filled with perilymph which communicates freely with that in the rest of the labyrinth. Within the membranous canals is the endo- lyniph, which communicates through the five openings with the endolymph in the utriculus. The canals lie in three planes that are, approximately at least, at right angles to one another (Fig. 170). The horizontal or external canals lie in a horizontal plane at right angles to the mesial or sagittal plane of the body, the vertical canals on each side make an angle of about 45 degrees with this mesial plane. The plane of each of the anterior canals is parallel to that of the posterior or inferior vertical canal of the opposite side, as represented in the figure. At one end of each canal near iis junction with the utriculus is the swelling known as the ampulla and within the ampulla lies the crista acusl tea containing the hair cells wilh which the nerve fibers communicate, and which, there- fore, are considered as the sense cells of the organ. cells are cylindrical gives off a long hair, perhaps of a bundl hairs, which projects interior of the canal for a distance of ai least 28 ,". The nerve hhers distributed to these hair cells are given off by the vestibular branch of the eighth nerve, or more properly the vestibular nerve, one branch of which (ramus utriculo-ampullaris) supplies the utriculus and the ampulla of the superior and horizontal canals, while the other (ramu jacculo-ampullaris) furnishes libers to the sacculus and the posterior ampulla. 394 Fig, I 76 I "' gram to how I - tion of the semicircular canals in the head bird. < >n «■--«.<- 1 1 i'l'- it n ill l" i ha) the three canal lie in plane at right angles to one anot hei . I he e ternal 01 mtal canal I E) <>f the i wo ide lie in the same plane, l he antei ior ca rial ol one ide A lie in a plane pa rallel tol hat i,f tli<- posterior canal (P) "I i he ot hei ide The hair and each consisting of liner into the SEMICIRCULAR CANALS AND THE VESTIBULE. 395 Flourens's Experiments upon the Semicircular Canals.— Modern experiments and theories concerning the functions of the semicircular canals date from the classical researches of Flourens* (1824). This investigator laid bare the canals in birds and mam- mals and studied the effects of sections of one or more of them. The experiments have since been repeated by numerous observers, and the results obtained have been described in great detail, for an account of which reference must be made to original sources.f In general, it may be said that injuries to the canals are followed by certain more or less definite movements of the head, eyes, and bod}', and by a disturbance in the power of the animal to co-ordinate nor- mally the muscles used in standing, locomotion, or flying. The character and extent of these results vary with the number of canals injured, and, indeed, show a more or less definite relationship to the several canals. When the horizontal canal is cut on one side in pigeons the animal makes movements of the head in the plane of that canal, and if the similar canal on the other side is also sec- tioned these movements are more pronounced. The animal may also in moving show an inability to walk normally and a tendency, especially when excited, to make abnormal forced movements of rotation of the whole body. After such an operation the pigeon will not fly voluntarily and if thrown into the air is not able to guide its flight with accuracy and soon descends. Similar operations on the anterior or the posterior canals cause movements of the head in the corresponding planes and a tendency in walking or flying to make forced movements — somersaults — forward or backward. When all three canals are cut on one or both sides the animal shows a distressing inability to maintain a normal position. The head is twisted, it is not able to stand unless supported, and any attempt at walking or flying results in violent forced and inco-ordinated movements. The animal makes continual somersaults at each attempt to stand or walk and the head is kept in spasmodic, forceful movements, which may produce injury or death. To preserve the animal from injury after such an extensive operation it is necessary to keep it wrapped in bandages. It should be added that results of this character are obtained only when the membranous canals are injured. If the bony canal alone is cut and even if the peri- lymph is removed by suction no such effects are obtained. At most slight and relatively transient movements of the head are observed. If the exposed membranous canal is pricked with a * Flourens, " Recherches experimentales sur les proprieties et les fonctions du systeme nerveux," second edition, 1842. fThe literature of the semicircular canals and the vestibule is very ex- tensive. The complete bibliography may be obtained from the following sources : " Die Lehren von den Funk'tionen der einzelnen Theile des Ohrlaby- rinths," by von Stein, 1894; Richet's " Dictionnaire de Physioiogie," article by Cyon, on "Espace," 1900. Ewald, 'Physiolog. Untersuchungen u. & Endorgan des nervus octavus," 1892. 396 THE SPECIAL SENSES. , needle more violent movements result . and if sectioned these move- ments are maintained for a longer period and are accompanied by the other results described. Similar effects have been obtained from operations on mammals and other animals, but the results are more pronounced in some animals than in others, varying apparently with the delicacy of the co-ordination necessary to the movements (Ewald). Thus, the movements of walking or flying in the pigeon may be assumed to require a nicer adjustment of the muscles used than is necessary in the swimming movements of the fish, and in correspondence with this idea it is found that opera- tions on the canals of fishes are not followed by conspicuous effects upon the movements of the animals. Temporary and Permanent Effects of the Operation. — The general effects of operations on the semicircular canals, so far as disturbances of equilibrium and occurrence of forced movements are concerned, resemble those resulting from operations upon the cerebellum, and, as in the case of the last mentioned organ, it is found by most observers that if the animal is properly cared for the severity of the first effects passes off to a greater or less extent. Flourens states that his pigeons, with two or more canals cut, continued to show the effects of the operation almost with the same intensity for nearly a year. Some unpublished experiments made in the author's laboratory have given different results.* Pigeons with only one canal cut recover practically completely within ten or more days. Those with two canals cut recover nearly completely within a month, so far as walking is concerned, although they exhibit an unwillingness to fly. Those with three or more canals cut never recover completely, but their final condition is very different from that exhibited shortly after the operation. Even when all six canals have been cut the animal, if well cared for in the beginning, is able finally to stand and walk and feed itself. It is not able, however, to fly, and in walking its progress is uncertain; there is a tendency to walk zigzag or in circles, first to one side, then to the other. If hurried or excited some return of the violent movements of the head and inco-ordination of the movements of locomotion may be seen. If, instead of cutting the canals, the ampullae are destroyed, the initial effects of the operation seem to be less violent, owing possibly to the fact thai in the former the irritative effects of the lesion still have the end organs in the ampulla? t'» acl upon. Pigeons with all six ampulla? destroyed may make eventually an excellent recovery. Within a few month- they walk and perch with little difficulty when not frightened. In the matter of flying they do qo1 recover their former skill, but this may be due to lack of practice, since • i |.m ni lasting over two year made by Dr. E. Rosencrantz, SEMICIRCULAR CANALS AND THE VESTIBULE. 397 in the experiments quoted (Rosencrantz) no provision was made for exercise in flying. The very marked degree of recovery noted, even after loss of all six ampullae, seems to be due to the fact that the animal learns to use his other sensory data in co-ordinating his muscles. If after a nearly complete degree of recovery has taken place a new operation is performed in which the canals are cut, the resulting disturbance to motion is relatively small and soon passes off. That there is any effect at all from the second operation may be due to the emptying of the endo- lymph and the consequent effect upon the remaining ampullae, or, if these had all been previously destroyed, to the effect upon the sense organs of the vestibular sacs. Effect of Direct Stimulation of the Canals. — The membranous canals or their ampullary enlargements have been stimulated by many observers and by many different methods — electrical, chemical, and mechanical. The results of electrical stimulation are not constant nor striking, but chemical and especially mechanical stimulation in the hands of many observers has called forth definite movements of head or eyes similar in a general way to those caused by section of the canal, but lasting, of course, for a short time only. In the dog-fish, Lee* finds that pressure upon an ampulla causes movements of the eyes and fins such as would occur normally if the animal's body were rotated in the plane of the canal stimulated. Effect of Section of the Ampullary or the Acoustic Nerve. — Many of the older and newer observers have cut one or both of the acoustic nerves or destroyed the entire labyrinth on one or both sides. The effects described vary somewhat with the animals used, but, in general, section of the nerve on one side is followed bjr forced movements, especially by rolling movements around the long axis of the body. When the nerves are cut on both sides disturbances in the power to maintain equilibrium perfectly are more or less distinctly marked. In fishes (dog-fish) the animal may swim or come to rest in unusual positions, — on the back or side, for instance. Is the Effect of Section of the Canals Due to Stimulation?— The movements that result from section of one or more of the canals have been attributed by some authors to stimulations set up by the injur}'- caused by the operation, and by others have been considered as a result of the falling out of the stimuli normally and constantly proceeding from the canals. This fundamental question has not been decided. On the one hand, the movements observed are simi- lar to those caused by excitation, which would indicate that a stimu- lation is set up by the operation. On the other hand, the effects are *Lee, "Journal of Physiology," 15, 328, 1903. 398 THE SPECIAL SENSES. so long lasting as to make it improbable that they are entirely due to the irritation of the operation. Moreover, Gaglio * states that when the spot operated upon is cocainized the same effects follow. Indeed, cocainizing the membranous canals gives the same results as cutting them. It is possible, of course, that both processes take place, an irritative stimulation and a falling out of normal impulses, the effects of the latter being longer lasting. Theories of the Functions of the Semicircular Canals. — As indicated briefly above, the facts regarding injury to and stimulation of the semicircular canals are very numerous and, on the whole, fairly concordant. Their interpretation, however, has offered great dif- ficulties, and man)' views have been proposed ; almost every inves- tigator, in fact, has, to some extent, varied in his interpretation of the precise functional significance of these organs, f These views may be classified, although imperfectly, under the following heads: 1. The old view, first proposed by Autenrieth (1802), that the canals or their sense cells are stimulated by sound waves and give us the means of determining the direction of sound in accordance with their position in three planes at right angles to one another. This view has been revived from time to time by recent writers. 2. Flourens himself believed that the impulses normally proceed- ing from these organs serve to moderate, or, as we should say now, to inhibit the movements of the head. As soon as the canals are cut the movements that have been kept under control by their influence are unrestrained. On this view the semicircular canals are organs which inhibit or restrain the voluntary movements, and thus take an essential part in the proper co-ordination of such movements. He did not attempt to define the physiology of the organs in terms of the sensations aroused. 3. The view that the stimulus to the hair-cells is to be found in the varying pressure of the endolymph. As first proposed by Goltz I 1870), it was assumed that the endolymph exerts a hydrostatic pressure upon the hair cells which in any given position varies in the different ampullas and varies with different positions of the head. The sensory impulses thus aroused give us a knowledge of the posi- tion of the lic.'nl .- 1 1 ) r 1 enable us, therefore, to control its movements and also those of the body. < »n this view these organs act as sense organs in maintaining body equilibrium and may be designated as peripheral sense organs of equilibrium. Later observers (Mach, Breuer, Brown, ei al. I modified this view by the assumption that the hair cells are stimulated not so much by the hydrostatic pressure of the endolymph as by I he pressure changes developed during move- glio, ■ \rrhivi-, ital. de biologie," 31, 377, L899. i I or :i detailed and complete account of these views to 1892, see Stein, "Die Leliren von den Funktionen der eizelnen Theile dea Ohrlabyrinths," Jena, L894 SEMICIRCULAR CANALS AND THE VESTIBULE. 399 ments of the head, making the organs, therefore, a means of appreciating especially the movements of the head, a dynamic rather than a hydrostatic organ of equilibrium. It was assumed that rotation movements of the head in the plane of a canal set up a movement or pressure of the endolymph in the opposite direc- tion, just as, to use a rough comparison, when one twirls a pail of water in one direction the water lags behind and exerts a pressure in the opposite direction. According to this hypothesis, which in some form or other is the view usually taught, the hair cells in each ampulla are stimulated chiefly by movements in the plane of that canal toward the ampulla, the pressure of the endolymph be- ing in the opposite direction, — that is, from utriculus toward the canal. Moreover, the vertical canals act in pairs (see Fig. 176), the superior or anterior vertical of one side acting with the posterior or inferior vertical of the other side, the two canals lying in parallel planes. Movements in this plane forward would stimulate the anterior ampulla on one side chiefly, movements in the same plane backward, the posterior ampulla of the opposite side. The horizon- tal canals also act together, being stimulated chiefly by rotational movements in the horizontal plane, the hair cells in one responding chiefly to movements in one direction, the other to movements in the same plane, but in the opposite direction. Rotational movements in other planes — sagittal, oblique, etc. — would affect two or more of the pairs of canals in proportion to the degree that each is involved in the movement on the principle of the parallelogram of forces.* By a mechanism of this sort it may be supposed that we are informed regarding the plane, di- rection, and extent of the movements of the head and are thereby enabled to control these movements. The canals function es- pecially as a dynamic organ of equilibrium, but may also give us guiding sensations when the movements are progressive rather than rotational, and also when the head is at rest, although, as is ex- plained below, this last function is by some relegated to the hair cells of the utriculus and sacculus. According to this view, the loss of the power of maintaining exact equilibrium after injuries to the canals or section of the nerves may be explained by supposing that false sensations are experienced and false compensatory move- ments are made. So, also, the vertigo experienced after continued rotation may be attributed to abnormal stimulation of these sense organs, — a view that finds some support in the fact that many deaf-mutes, whose internal ear is supposed to be deficient, do not experience vertigo after rotation, and in animals with the labyrinth destroyed rotational movements fail to give the symptoms of vertigo. * Consult Lee, loc. cit. 400 THE SPECIAL SENSES. 4. Cyon* has advocated the view that the semicircular canals constitute an organ for the perception of space in its three dimen- sions. Each canal or pair of canals gives us the sense of direction in its own plane, and the fact that we have three pairs in planes at right angles to one another gives the physiological foundation of our conception of space in three dimensions. On this fun- damental conception of space are projected the additional space conceptions derived from our visual, tactile, and muscle senses. This author is not specific in stating by what means the sensory cells in the three canals are stimulated. In addition to the sensations of direction and of space furnished by the canals, the nerve impulses from them are supposed to co-ordinate the action of the motor centers concerned in movements of the head and body. 5. Ewald, while accepting the general view that the sense cells are stimulated by the pressure of the endolymph, lays stress upon the fact that the nerve impulses thus aroused have, as their main result, a reflex effect upon the tonicity of the voluntary muscula- ture. The constant flow of impulses from these organs serves to maintain the muscles in a normal condition of tone. In animals with the labyrinth destroyed on both sides the body musculature is flabby and lacking in tonicity. On this view, therefore, the semi- circular canals constitute what might be called a muscle-tone organ, and the obvious disturbances in motion caused by their injury are due primarily to a diminution or loss in muscle tone, each canal possibly being reflexly connected with special muscles. Summary. — With reference to the kind of sensation mediated by the nerves of the semicircular canals, it should be borne in mind that these sensations are not distinctly recognized by consciousness; hence the difficulty of designating them by a specific name. Of the many qualities of sensation or consciousness which we can distinguish some have characteristics so clear that we recognize them at once and give them distinctive names, such, for instance, as the sensations of sight, hearing, taste*, etc. Others, however, produce a psychical reaction of such an indefinite character that they escape recognition by mere introspection. The change in con- sciousness is not sufficiently marked to make itself felt to the un- trained mind. This condition prevails regarding the sensations, if ;my, aroused through the semicircular canals; they are too indistinct to be recognized and named by an appeal to conscious- ness, and it would seem to be wiser to designate them after the analogy of the muscle sensations simply as semicircular canal sensations. Our perceptions or ideas of space and direction are * E. von 'yon, " Das Ohrlabyrinth ale organ der mathematischen Sinne fiir Raum und Zeit.," L908. SEMICIRCULAR CANALS AND THE VESTIBULE. 401 possibly founded in part upon these reactions and in part upon the muscle sense, visual, and tactile sensations. Our reasoning with regard to the semicircular canal sensations would be more satisfactory if it could be shown that the vestibular nerve after ending in the brain was continued forward by sensory paths to the cortex of the cerebrum. As a matter of fact, such paths have not been demonstrated, and if we assume that conscious sensations are mediated only through the cortex of the cerebrum we have no anatomical proof that the semicircular canals give us any reaction in consciousness. The vestibular nerve fibers end in the nucleus of Deiters and the nucleus of Bechterew, through which reflex connections are established with the motor centers of the spinal and possibly the cranial nerves. There is a connection also with the nucleus fastigii of the cerebellum and through this possibly with the cerebellar cortex, although this latter connection has not been actually demonstrated. With regard to the influence of the nerve impulses from the semicircular canals upon movements, all the facts known seem to indicate that they play an important part in the regulation or co-ordination of the movements of equilibrium and locomotion. Inasmuch as this gen- eral co-ordination or control seems to rest normally in the nervous mechanisms of the cerebellum and inasmuch as the vestibular nerves make possible end connections with the cerebellum, to- gether with the fibers of muscle sense, we may assume that the cerebellum forms the brain center in which the semicircular canal impulses exert their influence upon co-ordinated muscular con- tractions,— the cerebellum forms the nerve center for the semi- circular canals or the semicircular canals form a peripheral sense organ to the cerebellum. Some such hypothesis seems to be necessary to account for the general similarity between the effects of lesions of the canals and of the cerebellum. Whether the impulses from the canals are excitatory or inhibitory or both, as regards their effect upon muscular contractions, is not clearly apparent from the experimental evidence so far furnished, but Ewald's suggestion that they serve to maintain reflexly the tonus of the body musculature is perhaps the most acceptable view. In regard to the means by which these nerves are normally stim- ulated there is also much room for conjecture, but provisionally at least it seems permissible to adopt the view that variations in the pressure of the endolymph upon the hairs of the hair cells, especially in movements of rotation, constitute the immediate cause of their excitation. Granting that changes in position or movement of the head may cause such variations in pressure the theory offers a simple and satisfactory explanation of the mode of excitation and the means by which the excitation may vary 26 402 THE SPECIAL SENSES. appropriately under different conditions. While the endolymph theory may be criticized easily, no other equally satisfactory theory has been suggested to take its place. Functions of the Utriculus and Sacculus. — These small sacs contain sensory hair cells similar in general structure to those found in the crista of the ampullary sacs. The collection of hair cells with their supporting cells is designated as the macula, the macula utriculi and the macula sacculi. Lying among the hairs of the hair cell are found masses of small crystals of calcium carbonate, the otoliths or otoconia. In this respect the structure of the macula differs strik- ingly from that of the crista. The position and connections of the utriculus and sacculus lead at first naturally to the supposition that they are stimulated by the sound waves of the perilymph and are, therefore, concerned in the function of hearing. The accepted views regarding the functions of the cochlea in hearing make this organ sufficient for all auditory purposes and there is no specific part of this process that need be attributed to the vestibular sacs. It was, indeed, at one time suggested that their structure adapts them to respond especially to short and irregular vibrations, but no cogent reasons or facts have been advanced to support this view. The fact that the sacs are so closely connected with the semicircular canals suggests rather that the functions of these organs are similar and that like the canals, therefore, they influence the contractions of the muscles and function as organs of equilibrium. In recent years the view that has been most discussed is that ad- vanced by Breuer, — namely, that these organs give us information regarding the position of the head when at rest and when mak- ing progressive — that is, non-rotary — movements, supplementing, therefore, the functions of the semicircular canals on the supposition that these latter act especially in movements of rotation. Or, as it i- -"inetimes expressed, the sacs form a static and the canals a dy- namic organ of equilibrium. According to this view, the otoliths act as a means of mechanical stimulation of the hairs. Being heavier than the endolymph, they press upon the hairs with a force varying with the position of the head and thus give rise to sensations or reflexes which are adapted to the maintenance of equilibrium. Since the planes of the two sacs are different, they may be differ- ently affected by the same position or movement. So also in pro- gressive movements forward the weight of the otoliths may be im- agined to exercise a stress of some sort upon the hairs. This theory has been the subject of much investigation, numerous experiments having been made chiefly upon fishes and invertebrates.* Accord- * Consult the following papers: Sewall, "Journal of Physiology," 1, :»;;!), 1884; Lee, ibid., 15, 311, L893, :> n< I "American Journal of Physiology," I, 128, 1898; Lyon,' American Journal of Psychology, " 3, 86, L900. SEMICIRCULAR CANALS AND THE VESTIBULE. 403 ing to some observers destruction of these sacs or section of their nerves is accompanied by a distinct interference with the fish's nor- mal equilibrium: the animal swims at times upon its back or side and apparently loses its normal means of judging correctly its posi- tion. In many invertebrates there is present a sac, known as the otocyst, containing hair cells and otoliths. Its structure resembles that of the vestibular sacs of the mammalian ear, and it has been assumed that it has a similar function. Experiments by numerous observers have indicated that when the otoliths are removed the animal shows disturbances in equilibrium, particularly in the matter of the compensatory movements exhibited during rotation. Others, however, deny these facts and state that invertebrates without oto- cysts make compensatory movements when rotated and that in those with otocysts compensatory movements and maintenance of normal equilibrium persist after destruction of the sacs. A very ingenious experiment reported by Kreidl seems to show that the oto- liths may affect the hairs by their weight. When the palsemon, a crustacean, molts it casts off the inner lining of the otocyst, together with the otoliths. The otocysts in these animals lie at the base of the antennules and open freely to the exterior. After molting the animal by means of its claws places fine grains of sand in the otocyst to act as otoliths. Taking advantage of this peculiarity, Kreidl placed the animal, after molting, upon finely powdered iron, with the result that some of the iron granules were deposited in the oto- cyst in place of the usual grains of sand. When now a magnet was brought near to the animal reactions were obtained which showed that the pressure of the iron upon the hairs influenced its position. The position taken b}?" the animal under these conditions was such as would be expected as a resultant of the forces of magnetism and gravity, and the experiment, therefore, justifies the hypothesis that under normal conditions gravity affects the otoliths and through them the muscular co-ordination of the animal. These experiments have been confirmed by Prentiss.* This author has shown, moreover, that if larval lobsters (4th stage) are prevented from obtaining otoliths after moulting by placing them in filtered sea- water, their movements, like those of larvae deprived of their otocysts, show a distinct instability and lack of normal orientation. * Prentiss, 'Bulletin of Museum of Comparative Zoology," Harvard. 1901, xxxvi., No. 7. SECTION IV. BLOOD AND LYMPH. CHAPTER XXII. GENERAL PROPERTIES: PHYSIOLOGY OF THE CORPUSCLES. The blood of the body is contained in a practically closed system of tubes, the blood-vessels, within which it is kept circulating by the force of the heart beat. It is usually spoken of as the nutritive liquid of the body, but its functions may be stated more explicitly, al- though still in quite general terms, by saying that it carries to the tis- sues foodstuffs after they have been properly prepared by the diges- tive organs ; that it transports to the tissues oxygen absorbed from the air in the lungs ; that it carries off from the tissues various waste products formed in the processes of disassimilation ; that it is the medium for the transmission of the internal secretion of certain glands; and that it aids in equalizing the temperature and water contents of the body. It is quite obvious, from these statements, that a complete consideration of the physiological relations of the blood would involve substantially a treatment of the whole subject of physiology. It is proposed, therefore, in this section to treat the blood in a restricted way, — to consider it, in fact, as a tissue in itself, and to study its composition and properties without special reference to its nutritive relationship to other parts of the body. Histological Structure. — The blood is composed of a liquid part, the plasma, in which float ;i vast number of microscopical bodies, the blood corpuscles. There arc ;it least three different kinds of cor- puscles, known respectively as the red corpuscles or erythrocytes; the white corpuscles or leucocytes, of which in turn there are a number of different kinds; and the blood i>le set free indirectly by + ' + a secondary reaction. Thus Xa.,C03 dissociates into Na, Na = + - and C03, but the acid ion reacts with water, H, OH to form HC03 and OH. If we wish to know whether the blood or lymph has a true alkaline reaction, it is necessary to determine in them the proportion of hydrogen and hydroxyl ions. This has been done, with the result that they are found to exist in practically equal amounts, as in the case of pure water, and we must believe, therefore, that these liquids, which constitute the internal environment of the living cells, have neither a distinct acid nor alkaline reaction. When we examine the salts of the blood wc find that in addition to such neutral salts as sodium or potassium chloride there is present also a considerable amount of sodium carbonate, and one may ask why this latter salt does not give to the blood a real alkaline reaction in accordance with its behavior in aqueous solutions. The answer to this question is found in the fact that the liquids of the body are exposed continually to a certain pressure of carbon dioxid which is formed steadily in the tissues, on the one hand, and the excess of which is as steadily eliminated through the lungs, on (he the other hand. This carbon dioxid keeps the sodium carbonate in the form of a bicarbonate, which, so far as it is dissociated, + yields no hydroxyl ions, NaHC03 = Na, HC03, and therefore gives to the blood no actual alkalinity. When plasma or serum is treated with litmus-paper it gives an alkaline reaction, owing to the fact that the indicator, litmus acid, is ;i sufficiently strong- acid to combine with the base (Na) and drive it out, as it were, from its combination with carbonic acid. Phenolphthalein, being a weaker acid, does not displace 1 lie carbonic acid. If a relatively strong acid, such as acetic or tartaric, is added to the blood, it will unite with the base (Na) so far as this latter exists in combination with weaker acids, that is. in the liquid under consideration with carbonic or phosphoric acid. When, there- fore, 1 he blood is titrated with a standard solution of tartaric acid, it will continue to give a blue reaction with lit mus-pa per until all the base present in combination as carbonate or phos- phate has been united with the stronger acid. The amount of the standard acid used may be employed, therefore, to express the amount of base presenl in the blood in combination with such weak acids. For clinical and experimental purposes determinations of this kind are often made. Formerly the method was supposed to determine the alkalinity of the blood GENERAL PROPERTIES: THE CORPUSCLES. 407 on the assumption that the blue reaction of litmus is a true in- dication of alkalinity. While the process throws no light on the actual alkalinity of the blood, it does yield a valuable indication in regard to what may be called its potential alkalinity, that is, its power to neutralize acids added to it during the processes of normal or pathological metabolism, or under experimental conditions. The blood as it exists in the body contains always a certain amount of C02, NaHC03, Na2HP04, and also some sodium in combination with protein, which from this stand- point is to be regarded as a weak acid. It has been shown that to such a solution a considerable amount of acid or alkali may be added without altering its approximately neutral reaction. Since this property is due in large measure to the amount of base present in combination with weak acids, it is evident that the determination of this latter factor, that is, the "potential or titration alkalinity" of the blood, may be a matter of interest and importance.* Specific Gravity. — The specific gravity of human blood in the adult male may vary from 1.041 to 1.067, the average being about 1.055. The most satisfactory method of determining this factor is, of course, to compare the weight of a known volume of blood with that of an equal volume of water, but for observations upon human beings such small quantities of blood must be used that recourse must be had usually to a more indirect method. Perhaps the simplest of the methods suggested is that devised by Hammerschlag. f In this method a mixture is made of chloroform (sp. gr., 1.526) and benzol (sp. gr., 0.889). The mixture is made in such proportions as to have a specific gravity of about 1.055. A drop of blood from the finger is shaken into this mixture ; if the drop sinks to the bottom it is evident that the specific gravity of the blood is higher than that of the mixture, and the reverse is true if the drop rises. B}r adding more of the chloroform or of the benzol, as the case may be, the specific gravity of the mixture may be quickly altered so as to be equal to that of the drop of blood, which will then float in the liquid without a distinct tendency to rise or fall. The specific gravity of the mixture, which is also that of the blood, is then determined by a suitable hydrometer. By the use of such methods it has been found J that the specific gravity varies with age and with sex; that it is diminished after eating and is increased after exercise; that it has a diurnal variation, falling gradually during the day and rising slowly during the night; and that it varies greatly in individuals, so that a specific gravity which is normal for one may be a sign of disease * See Henderson, "American Journal of Physiology," 1908, 21, 427. | Hammerschlag, "Zeitschrift f. klin. Med.," 20, 444, 1S92. % See Jones, "Journal of Physiology," 12, 299, 1891. 408 BLOOD AND LYMPH. in another. The specific gravity of the corpuscles is slightly greater than that of the plasma. For this reason the corpuscles in shed blood, when its coagulation is prevented or retarded, tend to settle to the bottom of the containing utensil, leaving a more or less clear layer of supernatant plasma. Among themselves, also, the cor- puscles differ slightly in specific gravity, the red corpuscles being heaviest. Red Corpuscles. — The red corpuscles in man and in all the mammalia, with the exception of the camel and other members of the group Camelidae, are biconcave circular discs or, according to some authors, bell-shaped corpuscles without nuclei; in the Cam- elidae they have an elliptical form. Their average diameter in man is given as 7.7 in (1 ,u = 0.001 mm.); their number, which is usually reckoned as so many in a cubic millimeter, varies greatly under different conditions of health and disease. The average number is given as 5,000,000 per c.mm. for males and 4,500,000 for females. The red color of the corpuscles is due to the presence in them of a pigment known as "hemoglobin." Owing to the minute size of the corpuscles, their color when seen singly under the micro- scope is a faint yellowish red, but when seen in mass they exhibit the well-known blood-red color, which varies from scarlet in arterial blood to purplish red in venous blood, this variation in color being dependent upon the amount of oxygen contained in the blood in combination with the hemoglobin. Speaking generally, the func- tion of the red corpuscles is to carry oxygen from the lungs to the tissues. This function is entirely dependent upon the presence of hemoglobin, which has the power of combining easily with oxygen gas. The physiology of the red corpuscles, therefore, is largely con- tained in a description of the properties of hemoglobin. Condition of the Hemoglobin in the Corpuscle. — The finer structure of the red corpuscle is not completely known. It is usually stated that the corpuscle is composed of two substances, stroma and hem- oglobin, together with a certain amount of water and salts and also a certain amount of lecithin and cholesterin. The stroma is a delicate, extensible, colorless substance that gives shape to the corpuscles; it forms a meshwork or spongy mass in which the hemoglobin is deposited. This latter substance forms the chief constituent of the corpuscle, since it makes about 32 per cent, of the weight of the normal corpuscle;, and when dry from 90 to 95 per cent, of the total solid material. According to another view the corpuscles are vesicles with an external envelope or pellicle in which lecithin and cholesterin are found, while the hemoglobin is contained within.* Whichever view may be correct, great interest * For recenl discussions upon the histological structure of the corpuscles, see Weidenreich, "Anatom. Anzeiger," L905, xxvii., .r>s:j; Ruzicka, ibid., iii.. 153; Bchafer. ibid., L905, xxvi., 589. GENERAL PROPERTIES: THE CORPUSCLES. 409 attaches to the presence of the lecithin and cholesterin, whether these substances are found in an external membrane or in a stroma permeating the corpuscle. According to Pascucci the lecithin and cholesterin constitute as much as 30 per cent, of the dry weight of the stroma, that is, of the portion of the corpuscle left after re- moval of the hemoglobin. Such a large proportion of these two substances is not found elsewhere in the body except in the myelin sheath of the nerve fibers. It is believed that they play an impor- tant r61e in maintaining the integrity of the corpuscles and particularly in giving to the peripheral layer or membrane sur- rounding the corpuscles certain characteristic properties of permeability. Under normal conditions this external layer is easily permeable to water and to certain substances in solution, such as urea, alcohol, and ether, but it is said to be impermeable to the neutral salts; the concentration of sodium chloride, for example, is much greater in the plasma than in the red corpuscles. The condition in which the hemoglobin exists within the cor- puscle is not fully understood. It is evidently not in solution, since the amount present is too great to be held in solution in the corpuscle, and, moreover, even a thin layer of corpuscles is far from being transparent. Nor is it deposited in the form of crystals. It is assumed, therefore, that it is present in a peculiar, amorphous form, and Gamgee has shown that from its aqueous solutions the hemoglobin can be obtained in an amorphous state by the action of an electrical current. It is protected from the action of the water within and without the corpuscle. In various ways, however, the relations of the hemoglobin within the corpuscle may be disturbed; so that it escapes and enters into solution in the plasma. Blood in which this has happened suffers a change in color, becoming a dark crimson, and is, therefore, known as "laked blood." Laked blood in thin layers is quite transparent compared with the normal blood with its opaque corpuscles. Hemolysis. — The act of discharging the hemoglobin from the corpuscles so that it becomes dissolved in the plasma is designated as hemolysis, and substances that cause this action are spoken of as hemolytic agents. A number of such agents are known; but, although the results of their action are the same, so far as the hemo- globin is concerned, the way in which they bring about this result must vary greatly. Some of the known methods of producing hemolysis, or rendering the blood "laky," are as follows: (1) By the addition of water to the blood or by diminishing in any way the concentration or osmotic pressure of the plasma. (2) By add- ing ether or chloroform. (3) By the addition of soaps or of the higher fatty acids, especially the unsaturated acids. (4) By 410 BLOOD AXD LYMPH. adding bile or solutions of the bile-salts. (5) Ity adding amyl- alcohol. (6) By adding the serum from the blood of certain animals. (7) By adding saponin or sapotoxin. (S) By the addition of an excess of alkali. (9) By various toxins found in snake venom or in the serum of other animals or among the products of bacterial activity (natural hemolysins), or by similar organic substances produced within the body by the process of immunizing. Two of these methods demand especial men- tion, as they involve the consideration of processes of great physiological importance. Hemolysis Caused by Lowering the Osmotic Pressure of the Plasma. — The blood corpuscles contain a certain amount of water ( 57 to 64 per cent.), an amount insufficient to discharge the hemoglobin. We may imagine that the osmotic pressure within the corpuscle is such, compared with the osmotic pressure exerted by the salts in the plasma, that a water equilibrium is established, and that, although water molecules diffuse into and out of the corpuscle, the exchange is equal in the two directions. If, however, the outside plasma is diluted by the addition of water to any consider- able extent, then the osmotic pressure outside the corpuscles is correspondingly reduced, while that within the corpuscles is unchanged. Consequently an increased amount of water will pass into the corpuscles, sufficient, in fact, to vupture the cor- puscles and thus discharge the hemoglobin. It is evident, therefore, that in injecting liquids into the circulation or in diluting blood outside the body care must be taken not to use solutions whose osmotic pressure is markedly less than that of blood-plasma, otherwise many of the red corpuscles may be destroyed. Solutions whose osmotic pressure is the same as thai of the plasma are said to be isosmotic or isotonic with the blood, those whose pressure is lower are designated as hypotonic, and those whose pressure is higher as hypertonic.* The salt that is coni aiiicd iii the plasma in Largest amounts is sodium chlorid. In mammalian serum it exists to an amount equal to 0.56 per cent, and is probably responsible for the greater pari (60 percent.) of the osmotic pressure shown by this liquid. In making isotonic solutions this salt is, therefore, generaly employed. A solution containing nine-tenths of I per cent, of sodium chlorid (NaCl, ').!) per cent.) gives the same osmotic pressure as plasma as determined by the effect- of each on the lowering of the freezing-poinl (see Appendix, Diffusion, Osmosis, and Osmotic I 'res- 1 ire) . Such a solution mixed with blood * For .'i lull con ideration of osmotic pressure in its relations to physio- Logical proi Hamburger, "Osmotischer Druck und [onenlenre," Wiesbaden, 1902. GENERAL PROPERTIES: THE CORPUSCLES. 411 should not and does not alter the water contents of the corpuscles. One may, in fact, use a 0.7 per cent, solution of sodium chlorid without causing any noticeable hemolysis, and this strength of solution is frequently employed in infusions and experimental work; it constitutes what is known in the laboratories as nor- mal saline or physiological saline. If, however, one uses a lower concentration, some of the corpuscles are hemolyzed, and the number of corpuscles destroyed and the rapidity of the hemolysis increase rapidly with the lowering of the osmotic pressure. While a 0.9 per cent, solution of sodium chlorid suffices in most cases for infusions and for diluting blood, it does not entirely replace the normal plasma or serum, since these liquids, in addition to the sodium salts, contain salts of calcium, potassium, magnesium, etc., each of which has doubtless a certain specific importance. In diluting blood outside the body, when the dilution is large, better results are obtained by using what is known as Ringer's mixture, which consists of the physiological saline solution plus small amounts of potassium and calcium chlorid. One formula for Ringer's solution is : Sodium chlorid 0.9 per cent. Calcium chlorid 0.026 " " Potassium chlorid 0.03 " " Hemolysis Caused by the Action of Hemolysins. — It has long been known that the serum of one animal may destroy the red corpuscles of another animal. Thus, rabbits' blood corpuscles added to the clear serum of a dog, cat, or man are quickly destroyed, with the liberation of their hemoglobin. This action was formerly described under the term " globulicidal action of serum," and was compared to the similar destruction, bactericidal action, exhibited bjr serum toward some bacteria. In more recent literature the term hemol- ysis has replaced that of "globulicidal action," and the hemolytic effect that a serum may exert upon foreign corpuscles is attributed to the presence in it of certain substances which in general are classed as hemolysins. This hemolytic action is not due to a simple differ- ence in osmotic pressure. The serums of the different mammalia have all approximately the same osmotic pressure; the differences are too slight to explain the effects observed. Moreover, if the serum used is heated to 55° C. its hemolytic action is destroyed, although no noticeable change occurs in the osmotic pressure. In addition to the hemolysins found normally in the blood of different animals it was shown first by Bordet * that they may be produced artificially. The serum of guinea pigs has little or no effect normally on the red corpuscles of rabbits' blood. If, however, one injects * Bordet, "Annales de l'lnst. Pasteur," 1895. 412 BLOOD AND LYMPH. some rabbits' blood beneath the skin of a guinea pig and, if neces- sary, repeats the process it will be found that the blood of this particular guinea pig has now a strong hemolytic action toward the red corpuscles of rabbits. This method of producing specific hemolysins by means of subcutaneous or intraperitoneal injections of foreign red corpuscles is designated as a process of immunizing, and the serum of the animal in which a specific hemolysin has been thus produced is frequently called, for convenience, an immune serum. These terms are employed on account of the essential similarity of the processes involved to those underlying the devel- opment of immunity toward special diseases. When the body is in- vaded by pathogenic bacteria the toxic substances produced by these organisms stimulate the tissues to form specific antitoxins which are capable of neutralizing the action of the bacterial toxins. The body is thus rendered immune toward special bacteria, and that the blood of the immunized animal actually contains a definite anti- toxin may be shown in some cases by the fact that when injected into another individual the latter also acquires the specific immun- ity. So in regard to the hemolysins. The presence of the foreign red corpuscles causes the development of a specific antibody capable of destroying the special form of red corpuscle injected. This interesting reaction may be obtained with other cells than the red corpuscles and bacteria. By injecting spermatozoa a cytotoxin may be produced in the blood which destroys this particular form of cell, and the same fact holds good for epithelial cells, etc. Moreover, solutions of foreign proteins injected in the same way give rise to the formation of definite antibodies capable of coagulating or precipitating the special proteins used. In this last case the antisubstance is designated as a precipitin on account of its precipitating effect on the solution of protein (see Appendix, p. 967). This wonderful protective adaptation of the body toward the invasion of foreign cells or proteins is at bottom doubtless a chemical reaction dependent upon the properties of the living cells, but the nature of the proc- esses involved is not at all understood, and the phenomenon is, therefore, designated provisionally as a biological reaction. The specific hemolysins produced by immunization have been studied by Bordet, Ehrlich, and others.* It has been shown that they are in reality composed of two substances whose combined action is necessary lor 1 he hemolysis. There is, first, a new and specific Substance that is produced by the body as a consequence of the * For :> brief statemenl of the development of the subject, see Wasser- mann, "Immune Sera, Hemolysins, Cytotoxins, and Precipitins," trans- lated by Bolduan, New York, L904. For a more extended review, see Ase.liol'f, /,.,! chrifl f. allgemeine Physiologie, " L, 69, L902, or Ehrlich, "Collected Studies on Immunity, " translated by Bolduan, New York, L906. GENERAL PROPERTIES: THE CORPUSCLES. 413 injection of the foreign blood corpuscles. This substance has been given different names, but is known most frequently (Ehrlich) as the immune body (or amboceptor) . It is not destroyed by mod- erate heating. The immune body is enabled to act upon the corpuscles by the co-operation of certain substances which are normally present in the serum and are therefore not produced by the process of immunization. These substances are known usually as complements, and it is they that are destroyed by heating to 55° C. If the immune serum of a guinea pig is heated to 55° C. its hemolytic action upon rabbits' corpuscles is destroyed. The action may be restored, however, by adding a little of the rabbit's own serum, since in terms of the above hypothesis the complements are present in normal serum. That is to say, an experiment of the following kind may be performed. Washed blood corpuscles of a rabbit plus immune serum from a guinea pig show hemolysis. Washed blood corpuscles of a rabbit plus immune serum which has been made inactive by heating show no hemolysis. Addition of normal rabbits' serum to this latter mixture again activates the immune serum and causes hemolysis. The rabbits' serum in this case supplies the needed complement. These facts, it should be stated, are interpreted somewhat differently by Bordet. The immune substance he designates as a "substance sensibila- trice" and the complement as alexin. The latter forms the protective sub- tance of the blood, but is unable to act upon the foreign cells until these latter have been changed in some way, that is, sensitized by the specific immune substance developed during the process of immunizing. In the case of some of the natural hemolysins referred to above it has also been shown that the solution of the corpuscles depends upon the combined action of two substances. This point has been made clear particularly in regard to the snake- poisons, such as cobra venom. In these venoms there is present a substance analogous to the immune body or amboceptor, but in order for it to affect the red corpuscles it must be activated by a complement of some sort, present in the plasma or the red corpuscle itself. Kyes* has given some interesting facts to prove that lecithin is an effective complement for these venoms, and that probably it is this definite substance which is furnished by the blood in activating the venom toxin. The latter seems to form a definite compound with the lecithin, designated in the case of the cobra poison as a cobra-lecithid. Speaking in general terms, the serum of any animal is more or less hemolytic in relation to the blood corpuscles of an animal of another species; but great differences are shown in this respect. The blood-serum of the horse shows but little hemolytic action upon the red corpuscles of the rabbit when compared with the *Kyes, "Berl. klin. Wochenschrift," 1902 and 1903. 414 BLOOD AND LYMPH. effect of the serum of the dog or cat. Eels' serum has a re- markably strong hemolytic action upon the red corpuscles of most mammals; a very minute quantity of this serum (0.04 c.c.) injected into the veins of a rabbit will cause hemolysis of the corpuscles and, as a consequence, the appear- ance of bloody urine (hemoglobinuria). It should be added that this curious toxic or lytic effect of foreign serums is not confined to the red corpuscles. They contain cytotoxins that affect also other tissue elements, especially those of the central nervous system, and may therefore cause death. As little as 0.04 c.c. of eels' serum injected into a small rabbit will cause the death of the animal, the fatal effect being due apparently to an action on the vasomotor and respiratory centers in the medulla. The hemolytic and generally toxic effect of foreign sera has been known for a long time. It was discovered practically in the numerous attempts made in former years to transfuse the blood of one animal into the veins of another. It has been found that this process of transfusion as a means of combatting severe hemorrhage is dangerous unless the blood is taken from an animal of the same or a nearly related species. Nature and Amount of Hemoglobin. — Hemoglobin is a very complex substance belonging to the group of conjugated proteins. Under the influence of heat, acids, alkalies, etc., it may be broken up, with the formation of a simple protein, globin, belonging to the group of histons (see appendix) and a pigment, hematin. The globin forms, according to different estimates, from 86 to 94 per cent, of the molecule, and the hematin about 4 per cent. Other sub- stances of an undetermined character result from the decomposition.* When the decomposition takes place in the absence of oxygen, the products formed are globin and hemochromogen, instead of globin and hematin. Hemochromogen in the presence of oxygen quickly undergoes oxidation to the more stable hematin. Hoppe-Seyler has shown that hemochromogen possesses the chemical grouping which gives to hemoglobin its power of combining readily with oxy- gen and its distinctive absorption spectrum. On the basis of facts such as these, hemoglobin may be defined as a compound of a protein body with hematin. It seems, then, that., although the hemochro- mogen or hematin portion is I he essential constituent, giving to the molecule of hemoglobin its valuable physiological properties as a re piratory pigment, yet in the blood corpuscles this substance is incorporated into the much Larger and more unstable molecule of hemoglobin, whose behavior toward oxygen is different from that of the hematin itself, the difference lying mainly in the fact, that the hemoglobin as it exist,, in the corpuscles forms with oxygen a * SchuLz, 'Zeitschrift f. physiologische Chemie, " 24; also Lauraw, ibid., 26. GENERAL PROPERTIES: THE CORPUSCLES. 415 comparatively feeble combination that may be broken up readily with liberation of the gas. Hemoglobin is widely distributed throughout the animal king- dom, being found in the blood corpuscles of mammalia, birds, reptiles, amphibia, and fishes, and in the blood or blood corpuscles of many of the invertebrates. The composition of its molecule is found to vary somewhat in different animals; so that, strictly speaking, there are probably a number of different forms of hemo- globin— all, however, closely related in chemical and physiological properties. Elementary analysis of dogs' hemoglobin shows the following percentage composition (Jaquet): C, 53.91; H, 6.62; N, 15.98; S, 0.542; Fe, 0.333; 0, 22.62. Its molecular formula is given as C758H1203N195S3FeO218, which would make the molec- ular weight 16,669. Other estimates are given of the molecular formula, but they agree at least in showing that the molecule is of enormous size. The hematin that is split off from the hemoglobin is a pigment whose constitution is relatively simple, as is indicated by its percentage formula, C34H34N4Fe05 (Kiister). It contains all of the iron of the original hemoglobin molecule. Gamgee has called attention to two facts which seem to indicate that the globin and hematin do not exist as such in the hemoglobin molecule. Thus, hematin is magnetic, — that is, is attracted by a magnet, — while hemoglobin, on the contrary, is diamagnetic. Globin alone rotates the plane of polarized light to the left, levorotatory, while hemo- globin solutions are dextrorotatory. The exact amount of hemo- globin in human blood varies naturally with the individual and with different conditions of life.. According to Preyer,* the average amount for the adult male is 14 grams of hemoglobin to each 100 grams of blood. It is estimated that in the blood of a man weighing 68 kilograms there are contained about 500 to 700 grams of hem- oglobin, which is distributed among some 25,000,000,000,000 of corpuscles, giving a total superficial area of about 3200 square meters. Practically all of this large surface of hemoglobin is available for the absorption of oxygen from the air in the lungs, for, owing to the great number and the minute size of the capil- laries, the blood, in passing through a capillary area, becomes subdivided to such an extent that the red corpuscles stream through the capillaries, one may say, in single file. In circu- lating through the lungs, therefore, each corpuscle becomes exposed more or less completely to the action of the air, and the utilization of the entire quantity of hemoglobin must be nearly perfect. Instruments known as hemometers or hemoglobinometers have been devised for clinical use in determining the amount of hemoglobin in the blood of * "Die Blutkrystalle," Jena, 1871. 416 BLOOD AND LYMPH. patients. A number of different forms of this instrument are in use. In all of them, however, the determination is made with a drop or two of blood, such as can be obtained without difficulty by pricking the skin. The amount of hemoglobin in the withdrawn blood is determined usually by a colorimetric method, — that is, its color, which is due to the hemoglobin, is compared with a series of standard solutions containing known amounts of hemoglobin, or with a wedge of colored glass whose color value in terms of hemo- globin has been determined beforehand. For details of the structure of the several instruments employed and the precautions to be observed in their use reference must be made to the laboratory guides.* Compounds with Oxygen and other Gases. — Hemoglobin has the property of uniting with oxygen gas in certain definite propor- tions, forming a true chemical compound. This compound is known as oxyhemoglobin; it is formed whenever blood or hemoglobin solu- tions are exposed to air or are otherwise brought into contact with oxygen. Each molecule of hemoglobin is supposed to combine with one molecule of oxygen. According to a determination by Hiifner,t the O capacity of the Hb of ox's blood is 1.34 c.c. 0 to each gram of Hb. It should be stated that some observers J find that the maxi- mum oxygen capacity of the blood may show individual variations within narrow limits, and that, therefore, what we designate as hemoglobin may not be a single chemical substance, but a mixture of closely related compounds. Oxyhemoglobin is not a very firm compound. If placed in an atmosphere containing no oxygen it is dissociated, giving off free oxygen and leaving behind hemo- globin or, as it is often called by way of distinction, "reduced hemoglobin." This power of combining with oxygen to form a loose chemical compound, which in turn can be dissociated easily when the oxygen pressure is lowered, makes possible the function of hemoglobin in the blood as the carrier of oxygen from the lungs to the tissues. The details of this process arc described in the section on Respiration. Hemoglobin forms with carbon monoxid gas (CO) :i compound, similar to oxyhemoglobin, which is known ;,- carbon >n>>n. L30. j See Bohr, in Nagel'a "Handbucb der Physiologie, " vol. I. pt. i.. L905. GENERAL PROPERTIES: THE CORPUSCLES. 417 pound; the tissues of the body are thereby prevented from obtain- ing their necessary oxygen, and death results from suffocation or asphyxia. Carbon monoxid forms one of the constituents of coal-gas. The well-known fatal effect of breathing coal-gas for some time, as in the case of individuals sleeping in a room in which gas is escaping, is traceable directly to the carbon monoxid. Nitric oxid (NO) forms also with hemoglobin a definite compound that is even more stable than the CO hemoglobin; if, therefore, this gas were brought into contact with the blood, it would cause death in the same way as the CO. Oxyhemoglobin, carbon monoxid hemoglobin, and nitric oxid hemoglobin are similar compounds. Each is formed, apparently, by a definite combination of the gas with the hematin portion of the hemoglobin molecule, and a given weight of hemoglobin unites presumably with an equal volume of each gas. In marked contrast to these facts, Bohr* has shown that hemoglobin forms a compound with carbon dioxid gas, carbohemoglobin, in which the quantitative relationship of the gas to the hemoglobin differs from that shown by oxygen. In a mixture of O and C02 the latter gas is absorbed by hemoglobin solutions independently of the oxygen, so that a solu- tion of hemoglobin nearly saturated with oxygen will take up C02 as though it held no oxygen in combination. Bohr suggests, there- fore, that the 0 and the C02 must unite with different portions of the hemoglobin — the oxygen with the pigment portion and the C02 possi- bly with the protein portion. Although the amount of C02 taken up by the hemoglobin is not influenced by the amount of O already in combination, the reverse relationship does not hold in all cases. It is found that the presence of the C02 loosens, as it were, the combina- tion between the hemoglobin and the oxygen so that the oxyhemo- globin dissociates more readily than would otherwise be the case. This is observed at least when the oxygen is under a low pressure, such as occurs, for instance, in the capillaries of the tissues. The importance of this fact in regard to the oxygen supply to the tissues is referred to more explicitly in the section on Respiration. Presence of Iron in the Molecule. — It is probable that iron is quite generally present in the animal tissues in connection with nuclein compounds, but its existence in hemoglobin is noteworthy because it has long been known, and because the important property of combining with oxygen seems to be connected with the presence of this element. According to the analyses made, the proportion of iron in hemoglobin varies somewhat in different animals: the figures given are from 0.333 to 0.47 per cent. The amount of hemoglobin in blood may be determined, therefore, by making a * " Skandinavisches Archiv f. Physiologie," 3, 47, 1892, and 16, 402, 1904. 27 418 BLOOD AXD LYMPH. Y;^ • ill H-1 ^c> quantitative determination of the iron. The amount of oxygen with which hemoglobin will combine may be expressed by saying that one molecule of oxygen will be fixed for each atom of iron in the hemoglobin molecule. In the decomposition of hemoglobin into globin and hematin, which has been spoken of above, the iron is retained in the hematin. Crystals. — Hemoglobin may be obtained readily in the form of crystals (Fig. 177). As usually prepared, these crystals are really oxyhemoglobin, but it has , j .-, ^ been shown that reduced hemoglobin also crystallizes, although with more diffi- culty. Hemoglobin from the blood of different ani- mals varies to a marked degree in respect to the power of crystallization. From the blood of the rat, dog, cat, guinea pig, and horse, crystals are readily obtained, while hemoglobin from the blood of man and of most of the vertebrates crystallizes much less easily. Methods for preparing and purifying these crystals will be found in works on phys- iological chemistry. To ob- tain specimens quickly for examination under the mi- croscope, one of the most certain methods is to take some blood from one of the animals whose hemoglobin crystallizes easily, place it in a test-tube, add to it a few drops of ether, shake the tube thoroughly until the blood be- comes laky, — that is, until the hemoglobin is discharged into the plasma,- and then place the tube on ice until the crystals are deposited. Small portions of the crystalline sediment may then be removed to a glass slide for examination. According to Reichert, the deposition of the crystals is hastened by adding ammonium oxalate to the blood in quantities sufficient to make from 1 to 5 per cent, of the mixture. Hemoglobin from different animals varies not only as to the ease with which it crystallizes, but in some Crystallized hemoglobin (after f man; ry: Frey): a, b. Crystals from venous Mood c, from the Mood of a cat; '/, from the a guinea ijik; e, from the bl /, Irom the blood of a Sfjuirrcl }lood of jf a hamster; GENERAL PROPERTIES: THE CORPUSCLES. 419 cases also as to the form that the crystals take. In man and in most of the mammalia hemoglobin is deposited in the form of rhombic prisms; in the guinea pig it crystallizes in tetrahedra (d, Fig. 177), and in the squirrel in hexagonal plates. This difference in crystal- line form implies some difference in molecular structure, and taken together with other known variations in property shown by hemo- globin from different animals lead us to believe that the huge mole- cule has a labile structure, and that it may differ somewhat in its molecular composition or atomic arrangement without losing its physiological property of an oxygen-carrier. In this connection it is interesting to state that the hemoglobin of horses' blood, which crystallizes ordinarily in large rhombic prisms, may be made to give hexagonal crystals by allowing it to undergo putrefaction, and that the form of the crystals may then be changed from hexagons to rhombs by varying the temperature of the solutions.* The crystals are readily soluble in water, and by repeated crystallization the hemoglobin may be obtained perfectly pure. As in the case of other soluble protein-like bodies, solutions of hemoglobin are precipitated by alcohol, by mineral acids, by salts of the heavy metals, by boiling, etc. Notwithstanding the fact that hemoglobin crystallizes so readily, it is not easily dialyzable, behaving in this respect like non-crystallizable colloidal bodies. The compounds which hemoglobin forms with carbon monoxid (CO) and nitric oxid (NO) are also crystallizable, the crystals being isomorphous with those of oxyhemoglobin. Absorption Spectra. — Solutions of hemoglobin and its deriv- ative compounds, when examined with a spectroscope, give distinctive absorption bands. Light, when made to pass through a glass prism, is broken up into its constituent rays, giving the play of rainbow colors known as the spectrum. A spectroscope is an apparatus for producing and observing a spectrum. A simple form, which illustrates sufficiently well the construction of the appara- tus, is shown in Fig. 178, P being the glass prism giving the spectrum. Light falls upon this prism through the tube (A) to the left, known as the "colli- mator tube." A slit at the end of this tube (S) admits a narrow slice of light — lamplight or sunlight — which then, by means of a convex lens at the other end of the tube, is made to fall upon the prism (P) with its rays parallel. In passing through the prism the rays are dispersed by unequal refraction, giving a spectrum. The spectrum thus produced is examined by the observer with the aid of the telescope (B). When the telescope is properly focused for the rays entering it from the prism (P), a clear picture of the spectrum is seen. The length of the spectrum will depend upon the nature and the number of the prisms through which the light is made to pass. For ordinary purposes a short spectrum is preferable for hemoglobin bands, and a spectroscope with one prism is generally used. If the source of light is a lamp flame of some kind, the spectrum is continuous, the colors gradually merging one into another from red to violet. If sunlight is used, the spectrum will be crossed by a number of narrow dark lines known as the "Fraunhofer lines." The position of these lines in the solar spectrum is fixed, and the more distinct ones are * Uhlik, ''Archiv f. d. gesammte Physiologie," 104, 6-4, 1904. 420 BLOOD AND LYMPH. designated by letters of the alphabet, A, B, C, D, E, etc., as shown in the charts below. If while using solar light or an artificial light a solution of any sub- stance which gives absorption bands is so placed in front of the slit that the fight is obliged to traverse it, the spectrum as observed through the telescope will show one or more narrow or broad black bands that are characteristic of the substance used and constitute its absorption spectrum. The positions of these bands may be designated by describing their relations to the Fraun- hofer lines, or more directly by stating the wave lengths of the portions of the spectrum between which absorption takes place. Some spectroscopes are provided with a scale of wave lengths superposed on the spectrum, and when properly adjusted this scale enables one to read off directly the wave lengths of any part of the spectrum. When very dilute solutions of oxyhemoglobin are examined with the spectroscope, two absorption bands appear, both occurring in Fig. 178. — Spectroscope: P, The glass prism; A, the collimator tube, showing the slit, S, through which the light is admitted; B, the telescope for observing the spectrum. the portion of the spectrum included between the Fraunhofer lines D and E. The band nearer the red end of the spectrum is known as the "a-band"; it is narrower, darker, and more clearly defined th:m the other, the "/?-band" (Fig. 17!)). The width and distinct- of the bands vary naturally with the concentration of the solution used (see Fig. 180) or, if the concentration remains the same, with the width of the stratum of liquid through which the light With a certain minima,! percentage of oxyhemoglobin (let than 0.01 per cent.) the 0-band is bsl and the '/-band is very fainl in layers 1 centimeter thick. With stronger solutions the bands become darker and wider and finally fuse, while some of the extreme red end and a greal deal of the violet end of the sped ruin GENERAL PROPERTIES: THE CORPUSCLES. 421 are also absorbed. The variations in the absorption spectrum, with differences in concentration, are clearly shown in the accom- panying illustration from Rollett * (Fig. 180) ; the thickness of the layer of liquid is supposed to be one centimeter. The numbers on the right indicate the percentage strength of the oxyhemoglobin solutions. It will be noticed that the absorption which takes r> E b 660 650 640 630 620 610 600 5^0 580 S70 560 550 540 530 ,li,„l.ii.l„.Jiililiillli..ilmllinilililllmliMllili1lmili]lllli»lmilimlllii[i 510 500 MO Fig. 179. — Table of absorption spectra (Ziemke and Mutter) : 1, Absorption spectrum of oxyhemoglobin, dilute solution; 2, absorption spectrum of reduced hemoglobin ; 3, ab- sorption spectrum of methemoglobin, neutral solution; 4, absorption spectrum of met- hemoglobin, alkaline solution ; 5, absorption spectrum of hematin, acid solution; 6, ab- sorption spectrum of hematin, alkaline solution. place as the concentration of the solution increases affects the red-orange end of the spectrum last of all- Solutions of reduced hemoglobin examined with the spectroscope show only one absorption band, known sometimes as the " j'-band." This band lies also in the portion of the spectrum included between the lines D and E; its relations to these lines and the bands of * Hermann's " Handbuch der Physiologie," vol. iv., 1880 422 BLOOD AND LYMPH. oxyhemoglobin are shown in Fig. 179. The f-band is much more diffuse than the oxyhemoglobin bands, and its limits, therefore, especially in weak solutions, are not well defined. The width and distinctness of this band vary also with the concentration of the solution. This variation is sufficiently well shown in the accom- panying illustration (Fig. 181), which is a companion figure to the one given for oxyhemoglobin (Fig. 180). It will be noticed that the last light to be ab- sorbed in this case is partly in the red end and partly in the blue, thus explaining the pur- plish color of hemoglo- bin solutions and of venous blood. Oxy- hemoglobin soluti o n s can be converted to hem oglobin solutions, with a corresponding change in the spectrum bands, by placing the former in a vacuum or, more convenient ry, by adding reducing solu- tions. The solutions most commonly used for this purpose are am- monium sulphid a n d Stokes's reagent.* If a solution of reduced hemoglobin is shaken with ;iir, it quickly changes to oxyhemo- globin and gives two bands instead of <>nc when examined by the spectroscope. Any given solution may be changed in this way from oxyhemoglobin t<» hemoglobin, and the reverse, a. great number of times, thus demonstrating the facility with which hemoglobin takes \i\> and surrenders oxygen. Solutions of carbon monoxid hemoglobin also give a spec- .[ ! an ammoniacal solution of a ferrous salt, [tismade by dissolving 2 parts (by weight) of ferrous sulphate, adding 3 parts of tar- taric acid, and then ammonia to distincl alkaline reaction. A permanent precipitate bould not !><• obtained. aBC Fig. 180. — Diagram to show the variations in the absorption spectrum of oxyhemoglobin with varying concentration ..I the solution. — (After Rollett.) The numbers to the right give the strength of the oxy- jlobin solution in percentages; the letters give the positions of the Fraunhofer lines. To ascertain the amount of absorption for any given concentration up to 1 per cent., draw a horizontal line across the diagram at the level corresponding to the concentra- tion. Where ihi line passes through the shaded pari of the diagram absorption takes place, and the width of the absorption band i seen .-it once. The diagram shows dearly thai the amounl of absorption increases a the solution: become nunc concentrated, especially the absorption of the blue end of the spectrum. It will be noticed thai with concentrations between 0.6 and 0.7 pei cent, the two bands between D and £ f use into one. GENERAL PROPERTIES: THE CORPUSCLES. 423 trum with two absorption bands closely resembling in posi- tion and appearance those of oxyhemoglobin. They are dis- tinguished from the oxyhemoglobin bands by being slightly nearer the blue end of the spectrum, as may be demonstrated by observing the wave lengths or, more con- veniently, by super- posing the two spectra. Moreover, solutions of carbon monoxid hem- oglobin are not re- duced to hemoglobin by adding Stokes's liquid, two bands be- ing still seen after such treatment. A solu- tion of carbon mon- oxid hemoglobin suit- able for spectroscopic examination may be prepared easily by passing ordinary coal- gas through a dilute oxyhemoglobin solution for a few minutes and then filtering. Derivative Compounds of Hemoglobin. — -There are a number of pigmentary bodies which are formed directly from hemoglobin by decompositions or chemical reactions of various kinds. Some of these derivative substances occur normally in the body. The best known are as follows*: Methemoglobin. — When blood or a solution of oxyhemoglobin is allowed to stand for a long time exposed to the air it undergoes a change in color, taking on a brownish tint. This change is due to the formation of methemoglobin, and it is said that to some extent the transition occurs very soon after the blood is exposed to the air, and that, therefore, determinations of the quantity of hemoglobin by the ordinary colorimetric methods should be made promptly to avoid a deterioration in color value. Methemoglobin may be obtained rapidly by the action of various reagents on the blood, * For more detailed information concerning the chemistry and literature of these compounds, see Hammarsten, "Physiological Chemistry," translated by Mandel, fourth edition, 1904; Abderhalden, " Physiologische Chemie," 1906. Fig. 181. — Diagram to show the variations in the ab- sorption spectrum of reduced hemoglobin with vary- ing concentrations of the solution (after Rollett). The numbers to the right give the strength of the hemo- globin solution in percentages; the letters give the posi- tions of the Fraunhofer lines. For further directions as to the use of the diagram, see the description of Fig. 180. 424 BLOOD AND LYMPH. some of them oxidizing substances, such as permanganate of potash or ferricyanid of potash, some of them reducing substances. In- deed, it is known that the change may occur within the blood-vessels by the action of such bodies as the nitrites, antifebrin, acetanilid, etc. According to most observers, methemoglobin contains the same amount of oxygen as hemoglobin ; it is combined differently, however, forming a more stable compound, which can not be dis- sociated by the action of a vacuum. On this account, therefore, methemoglobin is not capable of acting as a respiratory pigment, and to the extent that it is formed in the blood this tissue suffers a loss of its functional value as a carrier of oxygen. By the stronger action of reducing solutions — such as ammonium sulphid — the oxygen may be removed from the methemoglobin and reduced hemoglobin be obtained. Methemoglobin crystallizes in needles, and its solutions give an absorption spectrum which varies ac- cording as the solution is neutral or has an alkaline reaction. In neutral solutions the characteristic band is one in the orange, as indicated in Fig. 179. In alkaline solution the absorption spectrum has three bands, two of which are nearly identical with those of oxyhemoglobin. Hematin (C34H34N4Fe05) is obtained when hemoglobin is de- composed by the action of acids or alkalies in the presence or oxygen. It may occur in the feces if the diet contains hemoglobin or hematin, or in case of hemorrhage in the stomach or small intestine, since both the pancreatic and the gastric secretion break up hemoglobin, with the formation of hematin. It is an amorphous substance, of a dark-brown color, easily soluble in alkalies or in acid alcoholic solu- tions. These solutions give a characteristic absorption spectrum which is represented in Fig. 179. Hemin (C34H3304N4FeCl) is regarded as the hydrochloric acid ester of hematin and is obtained by the action of HC1 upon blood previously treated with alcohol. The compound is obtained in the form of crystals, which under the microscope appear usually as small, rhombic" plates of a dark-brown color. These crystals may be obtained from small quantities of blood stains, etc., no matter how old, and they have been relied upon, therefore, as a sure and easy test for the existence of blood, — that is, hemoglobin. The test is oik- that has been much used in medicolegal cases, and may be carried out as follows: A bit of dried blood is powdered with a few crystals of Na('l. Some of the powder is placed upon a glass slide and covered with a cover-slip. By means of a pipette a drop or two of glacial acetic acid is run under the slip, and then by draw- ing the slide repeatedly through a flame the acid is evaporated to dryness, tal ing care not to heat the acid so high as to cause it to boil. After the evaporation of the acid water is run under the slip and the specimen is ready for examination with the microscope, GENERAL PROPERTIES: THE CORPUSCLES. 425 Hemochromogen (C34H3(.N4Fe05 ?) is obtained when hemoglobin is decomposed by acids or alkalies in the absence of free oxygen. By oxidation it is converted to hematin. Hemochromogen is crystal- line, and gives a characteristic absorption spectrum. Hematoporphyrin (C34H38N406) differs from the preceding deriv- atives of hemoglobin in that it contains no iron. It may be ob- tained from hematin by the action of strong acids, and is of much physiological interest because of its relationship to the bile pigments, which, like it, are iron-free derivatives of the hemoglobin. In old blood-clots or extravasations it has long been known that a colored crystalline product may be formed. This product was designated as hematoidin by Virchow and later was stated, on the one hand, to be identical with the bile pigment, bilirubin, and, on the other hand, to be isomeric with hematoporphyrin. Later observers have prepared from hematoporphyrin by careful reduction a substance designated as mesoporphyrin. It contains one less oxygen atom than the hematoporphyrin, and is claimed to be identical with hematoidin. Another fact of great general interest is that from plant chlorophyl there may be prepared a compound, phylloporphy- rin, very similar to the mesoporphyrin. It would appear from this relationship that the red coloring matter of the blood and the green coloring matter of plants are compounds that have some similarity in chemical structure. Histohematins. — This name is a general term that has been given to the coloring matter found in the tissues, so far as it has the property of taking up oxygen. The red coloring matter in some muscles is an example of such a compound and has been designated specifically as myohematin. According to most observers, myo- hematin is identical with hemoglobin, — that is, the muscle substance contains some hemoglobin, — and we may suppose that its presence in the tissue furnishes a further means for the transportation of oxygen to the muscle protoplasm. Bile Pigments and Urinary Pigments. — These pigments are referred to in the description of the composition of bile and urine. In this connection the fact may be emphasized that each of them is supposed to be derived from hemoglobin, and each constitutes, so to speak, a form of excretion of hemoglobin. Origin and Fate of the Red Corpuscles. — The mammalian red corpuscle is a cell that has lost its nucleus. It is not probable, there- fore, that any given corpuscle lives for a great while in the circulation. This is made more certain by the fact that hemoglobin is the mother substance from which the bile pigments are made, and, as these pigments are being excreted continually, it is fair to suppose that red corpuscles are as steadily undergoing disintegration in the blood- stream. 426 BLOOD AND LYMPH. The number of red corpuscles destroyed daily in the body has never been determined with any accuracy, but it may be quite large, as would appear from the following approximate calculation based upon our incomplete knowledge of the amount of bile-pigment secreted daily. From observations made upon cases of biliary fistulas in man it is estimated that the daily flow of bile amounts to about 15 gins, per kilogram of body weight. If we assume in accordance with the figures given by some authors that the bile contains as much as 0.2 per cent, of pigment, then 1.95 gms. of pigment will be secreted per i\ account of their small size and somewhat indefinite form the struct ure of the blood plot os is not sat isfact orily known. DeetjenJ * For ;i brief general discussion of opsonins, sec Hektoen, "Science," I eb i-'. 1909. fWrighl I'." ton Medical and Surgical Journal," June 7, L906) calls attention i<> :i relationship between the blood-plates ami the giant cells of ili.' marrow (megalokaryocytes), and ventures the opinion thai the plates are detached pieces "I the cytoplasm of Hie jri.-mi cells. f'Virchow's Archiv f. path. Anat. u. Physiol.," 164, 239, L901. GENERAL PROPERTIES: THE CORPUSCLES. 433 has demonstrated that they are capable of ameboid movements. When removed from the blood vessels to a glass slide they usually agglutinate into larger or smaller masses, swell, and disintegrate, but if received upon a surface of agar-agar which has been made up with physiological saline, together with some sodium metaphos- phate (NaPOs), they flatten out, show a central granular portion and a peripheral clear layer, and may make quite active ameboid movements. Deetjen claims also that they possess a distinct nucleus. This latter statement is perhaps doubtful, as other observers report that the material which stains like a nucleus is present as separate granules in the interior of the plate. These granules, though doubtless of nuclear material, do not have the morphological appearance of a cell nucleus. It remains, therefore, uncertain whether the blood plates are to be considered as inde- pendent cells or as fragments of disintegrated cells. On account of their tendency to agglutinate and dissolve when the blood is shed it is difficult to obtain reliable data as to their numbers under normal and pathological conditions.* The results obtained by later observers using special methods to prevent known sources of error indicate that the average number may be 500,000 per cubic millimeter. The extremes reported vary from 200,000 or 250,000 to 778,000. Under certain pathological conditions, especially in pernicious anemia and lymphatic leukemia, their number is greatly reduced,' while in the acute infectious diseases there is said to be a diminution in number during the period of fever, followed by a marked increase beyond the normal during the period of convalescence. Outside the part that they take in the formation of thrombi and in the initiation of coagulation, nothing definite is known of their function under normal conditions. * For a summary of the literature and methods, consult Kemp, "Journal of the American Medical Association," April 7 and 14, 1906, and Pratt, ibid., Dec. 30, 1905. The preservative solution recommended by Pratt consists of sodium metaphosphate, 2 grams; sodium chlorid, 0.9 gram; water, 100 c.c. That preferred by Kemp is, formalin (40 per cent, aqueous solution of for- maldehyd), 10 c.c; sodium chlorid (1 per cent, solution), 150 c.c. 28 CHAPTER XXIII. CHEMICAL COMPOSITION OF THE BLOOD-PLASMA; CO- AGULATION; QUANTITY OF BLOOD; REGENERA- TION AFTER HEMORRHAGE. Composition of the Plasma and Corpuscles. — Blood (plasma and corpuscles) contains a great variety of substances, as might be inferred from its double relations to the tissues as a source of nutrition and as a means of removing the waste products of their functional activity. The constituents that may be present in normal blood-plasma are in part definitely known and in part entirely unknown from a chemical standpoint. Some idea of the complexity of the composition may be obtained from the following table : COMPOSITION OF THE BLOOD-PLASMA. Water, Oxygen, Carbon Dioxid, Nitrogen. f Fibrinogen. Protein, \ Paraglobulin { Scrum-albumin Euglobulin. Pseudoglobulin. Extractives, — that is, substances other than proteins that may be ex- tracted from the dried residue by water, alcohol, or ether. Salts of Nucleo-protcin. f Fats. Sugar. I Urea. j Jecorin. Glucuronic acid. Lecithin. Cholostorin Lactic acid. Chi., rids Carbonates Sulphates Phosphates Internal secretions. Tii f Lipase. Enzymes j GryColMe> eto Immune bodies (Amboceptors). I Complements. [ Opsonins. A number of detailed chemical analyses of the blood of different animals, so far as its constituents can be determined by analytical methods, have been reported at different times. The following 444 Enzymes and unknowns. Sodium. Potassium. Calcium. Magnesium Iron. CHEMICAL COMPOSITION OF BLOOD-PLASMA. 435 table, taken from Abderhalden,* and showing the composition of dogs' blood, may serve as an example: 1000 Parts, by 1000 Parts, by 1000 Parts, by Weight, of Blood Weight, of Se- Weight, of Corpus- Contain rum Contain cles Contain Water 810.05 923.98 644.26 Solids 189.95 76.02 355.75 Hemoglobin 133.4 327.52 Protein 39.68 60.14 9.918 Sugar... 1.09 1.82 Cholesterin 1.298 0.709 2.155 Lecithin 2.052 1.699 2.568 Fat 0.631 1.051 Fatty acids 0.759 1.221 0.088 Phosphoric acid: as nuclein 0.054 0.016 0.110 Na^O 3.675 4.263 2.821 K20 0.251 0.226 0.289 Fe203 0.641 1.573 CaO 0.062 0.113 MgO 0.052 0.040 0.071 CI 2.935 4.023 1.352 P205 0.809 0.242 1.635 Inorganic : P205 0.576 0.080 1.298 The same constituents in much the same proportions are found in the blood of all the mammalia examined. The amount of protein in the serum is greater in some cases than in others, — in the dog, for instance, according to Abderhalden's analyses, the protein amounts to only 6 per cent., while in the horse it may be 7 or 8 per cent. So also there are small variations in the amount of choles- terin, sugar, and other constituents, but, on the whole, the composi- tion of the liquid part of the blood, blood-serum or blood-plasma, is remarkably uniform so far as chemical analyses go. We know, however, that the physiological properties of mammalian serum may be very different indeed; that the serum of a dog, for instance, will kill a rabbit when injected into its vessel. Such physiological dif- ferences as this, however, depend upon constituents which can not be defined or determined by chemical means. The chemical com- position of the blood-serum differs from that of the red corpuscles in a number of respects in addition to the presence of hemoglobin in the latter. The corpuscles contain no sugar nor fat, a larger amount of cholesterin, lecithin, phosphoric acid and potassium, and less sodium and chlorin. The red corpuscles of different mammalia show a remarkable variation in the amount of potassium salts contained. Thus, according to Brandenburg, 1000 parts by weight of the red corpuscles contain the following amounts of K20 in different mammalia: Cat, 0.258: dog, 0.257; man, 4.294; horse, 4.957; rabbit, 5.229. * "Zeitschrift f. physiologische Chemie," 25, 88, 1S98. 436 BLOOD AND LYMPH. Proteins of the Blood-plasma. — The general properties and reactions of proteins and the related compounds, as well as a classi- fication of those occurring in the animal body, are described briefly in the Appendix. This description should be read before attempt- ing to study the proteins of the plasma and the part they take in coagulation. Three proteins are usually described as existing in the plasma of circulating blood, — namely, fibrinogen, paraglobulin, or, as it is sometimes called, "serum-globulin," and serum-albumin. The first two of these proteins, fibrinogen and paraglobulin, belong to the group of globulins, and hence have many properties in com- mon. Serum-albumin belongs to the group of albumins, of which egg-albumin constitutes another member. Serum-albumin. — This substance is a typical protein. It can be obtained readily in crystalline form from the horse's blood. Its percentage composition, according to Michel, is as follows: C, 53.08; H, 7.10; N, 15.93; S, 1.90; O, 21.96. Its molecular composition, according to Schmiedeberg,* may be represented by C78H122N20SO24 or some multiple of this formula. Serum-albumin shows the general reactions of the native albumins. One of its most useful reactions is its behavior toward magnesium sulphate and ammonium sulphate. Serum-albumin usually occurs in the body-liquids together with the globulins, as is the case in blood. If such a liquid is thoroughly saturated with solid magnesium sul- phate or half saturated with ammonium sulphate, the globulins are precipitated completely, while the albumin is not affected. So far as the blood and similar liquids are concerned, a definition of serum-albumin might be given by saying that it comprises all the proteins not precipitated by saturation with magnesium sul- phate or by half saturation with ammonium sulphate. When its solutions have a neutral or an acid reaction, serum-albumin is precipitated in an insoluble form by heating the solution above a certain degree. Precipitates produced in this way by heating solutions of proteins are spoken of as coagulations — heat coagula- tions— and the exact temperature at which coagulation occurs is to a certain extent characteristic for each protein. The tem- perature of coagulation of serum-albumin is usually given at from 70° to 75° C, but it varies greatly with the conditions. — for in- stance, with the reaction of the solution, its concentration in salts, or with the nature of the; salts present. It has been asserted, in fact, thai careful heating under proper- conditions gives separate coagulations at, three different temperatures, namely, 73°, 77°, and S4° C, — indicating the possibility that whal is called "serum- albumin" may be a mixture, of three proteins. Serum-albumin occurs in blood-plasma and blood-serum, in lymph, and in the different normal and pathological exudations found in the body; * "Archiv f. exper. Pathol u, Pharmakol.," 39, 1, 1897. CHEMICAL COMPOSITION OF BLOOD-PLASMA. 437 such as pericardial liquid, hydrocele fluid, etc. The amount of serum-albumin in the blood varies in different animals, ranging among the mammalia from 2.67 per cent, in the horse to 4.52 per cent, in man. In some of the cold-blooded animals it occurs in surprisingly small quantities, — 0.36 to 0.69 per cent. As to the source or origin of serum-albumin, it is frequently stated that it comes from the digested proteins of the food. It is known that protein material in the food is not changed at once to serum-albumin during the act of digestion; indeed, it is known that the final products of digestion are a group of proteins of an entirely different char- acter,— namely, peptones and proteoses, — or, indeed, a series of much simpler split products; but during the act of absorption into the blood these latter bodies have been supposed to undergo transformation into serum-albumin. From a physiological stand- point serum-albumin is often considered to be the main source of protein nourishment for the tissues generally. As will be explained in the section on Nutrition, one of the most important requisites in the nutrition of the cells of the body is an adequate supply of protein material to replace that used up in the chemical changes, the metabolism, of the tissues. Serum-albumin has been supposed to furnish a part, at least, of this supply, although, as a matter of fact, there is no substantial proof that this view is correct. As long as the serum-albumin is in the blood-vessels it is, of course, cut off from the tissues. The cells, however, are bathed directly in lymph, and this in turn is formed from the plasma of the blood which is transuded or, according to some physiologists, secreted through the vessel walls. Paraglobulin, which belongs to the group of globulins, exhibits the general reactions characteristic of the group. As stated above, it is completely precipitated from its solutions by saturation with magnesium sulphate or by half saturation with ammonium sulphate. It is incompletely precipitated by saturation with common salt (NaCl). In neutral or feebly acid solutions it coagulates upon heating to 75° C. Hammarsten gives its percentage composition as: C, 52.71; H, 7.01; N, 15.85; S, 1.11; 0, 23.32. Schmiedeberg gives it a molecular composition corresponding to the formula C117H182N30SO38 + iH20. According to Faust, the precipitate of paraglobulin usually obtained with magnesium sulphate contains a certain amount of an albuminoid body, glutolin, which he believes to be a constant constituent of blood-plasma. Paraglobulin occurs in blood, in lymph, and in the normal and pathological exudations. The amount of paraglobulin present in blood varies in different animals. Among the mammalia the amount ranges from 1.78 per cent, in rabbits to 4.56 per cent, in the horse. In human blood it is given at 3.10 per cent., being less in amount, therefore, than the serum-albumin. It is usually stated that more of this protein is 438 BLOOD AND LYMPH. found in the serum than in the plasma. This fact is explained by supposing that during coagulation some of the leucocytes disinte- grate and part of their substance passes into solution as a globulin identical with or closely resembling paraglobulin. Paraglobulin as obtained from blood-serum by half saturation with ammonium sulphate or full saturation with magnesium sulphate does not behave like a chemical individual. Portions of it, for instance, are precipi- tated by CO, or by dialysis, and portions are not so precipitated. Recently, therefore, it has been assumed that paraglobulin is in reality a mixture of two or possibly three different, although re- lated, proteins. The separation usually given is into euglobulin and pseudoglobulin, euglobulin being the portion precipitated by ammonium sulphate when added to one-third saturation (28 to 33 per cent.), and pseudoglobulin the portion precipitated only by one-half saturation (34 to 50 per cent.). The latter portion shows properties more nearly related to the albumins.* The whole basis of classification is, however, unsatisfactory and provisional (see appendix). It is even stated that under certain conditions of temperature and reaction serum-albumin may be converted to a globulin body that precipitates upon one-half saturation with ammonium sulphate.f The origin of paraglobulin remains unde- termined. It may arise from the digested proteins absorbed from the alimentary canal, but there is no evidence to support such a view. Another suggestion is that it comes from the disintegration of the leucocytes (and other formed elements) of the blood. These bodies are known to contain a small quantity of a globulin resem- bling paraglobulin, and it is possible that this globulin may be liber- ated after the dissolution of the leucocytes in the plasma, and thus go to make up the normal supply of paraglobulin. Several observers^ have claimed that during starvation the proportion of globulins in the blood is increased relatively or absolutely. A possible explanation is that the increase is due to cell globulins received from the tissues which must undergo destruction and dissolution in pro- longed fasting. The fact remains, however, that our knowledge is too incomplete at present to venture any positive statements regarding the origin and specific functions of the paraglobulin. Fibrinogen is a protein belonging to the globulin class and exhibit- ing all the general reactions of this group. It is distinguished from paraglobulin by a number of special reactions; for example, its temperature of heat coagulation is much lower (56° to 60° C), and * Por^es and Spiro, "Beitrage zur chem. Physiol, u. Pathol.," 3, 277, 1903; and Freund and Joachim, "Zeitechrift f. physiolotfische Chemie," 36, 407, 1002. fMoll, "Beitrage zur chem. Physiol, u. Pathol. " 4, 561, 1003. I Bee St. <;itlH-.,s, " lii-Wriifrt- zur diem. Physiol, u. Pathol.," 5, 515, 1904; ftluo Pcwin.ski, "Pfluger'a ArchlV f. d. gesammte Physiol.," 100, 611, 1903. CHEMICAL COMPOSITION OF BLOOD-PLASMA. 439 it is completely thrown down from its solutions by saturation with sodium chlorid as well as with magnesium sulphate. Its most important and distinctive reaction is, however, that under proper conditions it gives rise to an insoluble protein, fibrin, whose forma- tion is the essential phenomenon in the coagulation of blood. Fibrinogen has a percentage composition, according to Hammar- sten, of: C, 52.93; H, 6.90; N, 16.66; S, 1.25; 0, 22.26; while its molecular composition, according to Schmiedeberg, is indicated by the formula C108H162N30SO34. Fibrinogen is found in blood-plasma, lymph, and in some cases, though not always, in the normal and pathological exudations. It is absent from blood-serum, being used up during the process of clotting. It occurs in very small quantities in blood, compared with the other proteins. There is no good method of determining quantitatively the amount of fibrinogen, but estimates of the amount of fibrin, which cannot differ very much from the fibrinogen, show that in human blood it varies from 0.22 to 0.4 per cent. In horse's blood it may be more abundant, — 0.65 per cent. As to the origin and the special physiological value of this protein we are, if possible, more in the dark than in the case of paraglobulin, with the exception that fibrinogen is known to be the source of the fibrin of clotted blood. But clotting is an occasional phenomenon only. What nutritive function, if any, is possessed by fibrinogen under normal conditions is unknown. No satisfactory account has been given of its origin. It has been suggested by different investigators that it may come from the nuclei of disintegrating leucocytes (and blood plates) or from the dissolution of the extruded nuclei of newly made red corpuscles, but here again we have only speculations, that can not be accepted until some experimental proof is advanced to support them. It may be added that there is some evidence to indicate that the fibrinogen is produced in the liver. Thus Doyon and his co-workers * have shown that when the fibrinogen is re- moved from blood by defibrinating and the blood is then returned to the animal, new fibrinogen is produced within a few hours. In frogs, at least, the new fibrinogen is not manufactured if the liver is removed, and in dogs it is also stated that after removal of the liver the blood may become incoagulable owing to a disap- pearance of fibrinogen. The following table f gives some recent results of analyses of blood which indicate the average amounts of the different proteins in the blood-plasma of several animals. The figures give the weight of the protein in grams for 100 c.c. of plasma. * Doyon et al., "C. R. Soc. de Biol.,'; lx., 606, 681, 860. See also Nolf, ''Archives internat. de Physiol.," 1905, hi., 1. f Lewinski, loc. r.it. 440 BLOOD AND LYMPH. Serum- Total Proteins. Albumin. Paraglobulin. Fibrinogen. Man 7.26 4.01 2.83 0.42 Dog 6.03 3.17 2.26 0.60 Sheep 7.29 3.83 3.00 0.46 Horse .-.8.04 2.S0 4.79 0.45 Pig S.05 4.42 2.98 0.65 Otlier Proteins of the Blood-serimi or Blood-plasma. — From time to time other protein bodies have been described in the serum or plasma of the blood. In the serum after coagulation Hammarsten has obtained a globulin body, Hbrin-globulin. which he supposes may be split off from the fibrinogen during the act of clotting. Faust, as was mentioned above, describes an albuminoid substance, glutolin, which is present in the blood and is usually precipitated together with the paraglobulin. A number of observers have noted the ex- istence in blood of a protein not coagulated by heat. By some authors this has been described as a peptone or an albumose (Langstein), by others as an ovomucoid (Zanetti), and by others still (Chabrie) as a peculiar protein for which the name albumon has been proposed. By others still this non-coag[U- lable protein obtained from serum or plasma has been explained as an artificial product arising from the globulins of the blood during the process of remov- ing the coagulable proteins by heating. So, too, nucleoprotein substances have been described in the blood-serum by several observers, most recently by Freund and Joachim. It is quite possible, however, that the substance de- scribed as nucleoprotein is in reality a mixture or combination of lecithin and protein. Most of the protein when precipitated from the blood carries down with it some lecithin, and will therefore show a reaction for phosphorus. It can be shown that the phosphorus present is, in most cases at least, remov- able by boiling with alcohol, and there is at present no entirely satisfactory proof that nucleoprotein exists in the blood. Coagulation of Blood. — One of the most striking properties of blood is its power of clotting or coagulating shortly after it escapes from the blood-vessels. The general changes in the blood during this process are easily followed. At first perfectly fluid, in a few minutes it becomes viscous and then sets into a soft jelly which quickly becomes firmer, so that the vessel containing it may be inverted without spilling the blood. The clot continues to grow more compact and gradually shrinks in volume, pressing out a smaller or larger quantity of a clear, faintly yellow liquid to which the name blood-serum is given. The essential part of the clot is the fibrin, fibrin is an insoluble protein not found in normal blood, in shed blood, and under certain conditions in blood while still in the blood-vessels, this fibrin is formed from the soluble fibrinogen. The deposition of the fibrin is peculiar. It is precipitated, if the word may be used, in the form of an exceedingly fine network of delicate I hreada which permeate the whole mass of the blood and give the elol its jelly-like character. The shrinking of the threads causes the subsequent contraction of (lie clot. If the blood has not been disturbed during the act of clotting, the red corpuscles are caught in the line fibrin meshwork, and as the clot shrinks these corpuscles are held more firmly, only the clear liquid of the blood being squeezed out, so that ii is possible to get specimens of serum containing few or no red corpuscles. The; leucocytes, on the con- COAGULATION. 441 trary, although they are also caught at first in the forming meshwork of fibrin, may readily pass out into the serum in the later stages of clotting, on account of their power of making ameboid movements. If the blood has been agitated during the process of clotting, the delicate network will be broken in places and the serum will be more or less bloody — that is, it will contain numerous red corpuscles. If during the time of clotting the blood is vigorously whipped with a bundle of fine rods, all the fibrin is deposited as a stringy mass upon the whip, and the remaining liquid part consists of serum plus the blood corpuscles. Blood that has been whipped in this way is known as " defibrinated blood." It resembles normal blood in appearance, but is different in its composition; it can not clot again. The way in which the fibrin is normally de- posited may be demonstrated very easily under the microscope by placing a good-sized drop of blood on a slide, covering it with a cover-slip, and allowing it to stand for several minutes until coagu- lation is completed. If the drop is now examined, it is possible by careful focusing to discover in the spaces between the masses of corpuscles many examples of the delicate fibrin network. The physiological value of clotting is that it stops hemorrhages by closing the openings of the wounded blood-vessels. Time of Clotting. — The time necessary for the clot to form varies slightly in different individuals, or in the blood of the same in- dividual varies with the conditions. It may be said in general that under normal conditions the blood passes into the jelly stage in from three to ten minutes at room temperature (20° C). The separation of clot and serum takes place gradually, but is usually completed in from ten to forty-eight hours. The time of clotting shows marked variations in different animals; the process is especially slow in the blood of the horse, terrapin, and birds, so that coagulation of shed blood is more easily prevented in these animals. In the human being also the time of clotting may be much prolonged under certain conditions — in fevers, for example. This fact was noticed in the days when blood-letting was a common practice. The slow clotting of the blood permitted the red corpuscles to sink somewhat, so that the upper part of the clot in such cases was of a lighter color, forming what was called the "buffy coat." The time of clotting may be shortened or prolonged, or the clotting may be prevented altogether, in various ways, and much use has been made of this fact in study- ing the composition and the coagulation of blood as well as in controlling hemorrhages.* * For clinical methods of determining the coagulation time with a drop or two of blood, reference may be made to the manuals of clinical diagnosis. For a recent paper, see Addis, "Quarterly Journal of Exp. Physiology," 1908, I, 305. 442 BLOOD AND LYMPH. Theories of Coagulation. — The clotting of blood is such a prom- inent phenomenon that it has attracted attention at all times, and as a result numerous theories to account for it have been advanced. Most of these theories have now simply an historical interest. In recent years much experimental work has been done upon the subject, the result of which has been to increase greatly our knowledge of the process ; but no complete explanation has yet been reached. It is generally admitted that the essential constit- uent of the clot — namely, the fibrin — is formed from the fibrinogen normally present in the plasma, and that without this fibrin- ogen clotting is impossible. If, for instance, blood is heated to 60° C, a temperature sufficient to precipitate the fibrinogen as a heat coagulum, its power of clotting is lost. Clotting, therefore, is essentially a process of the blood-plasma, as was shown indeed by the old experimenters (Hewson). Moreover it is also admitted that the conversion of the soluble fibrinogen to the insoluble fibrin is accomplished by the agency of a substance, known as thrombin or fibrin ferment, which is not present, in its active form at least, in the blood while in the blood-vessels, but is formed after the blood is shed or under certain abnormal conditions within the blood-vessels. These two important facts we owe mainly to the investigations of Alexander Schmidt,* whose work completed the older observations of Hewson, Buchanan, Denis, and Briicke. Preparation of Solutions of Fibrinogen.! — Fibrinogen may be obtained readily in solution free from other proteins by a modi- fication of the general method first described by Hammarsten. One may use the plasma of horse's blood which has been kept from clotting by prompt cooling, and in which the corpuscles have been thrown down by centrifugalizing or by long standing at low temperature, but it is more convenient, perhaps, to use cat's blood which has been kept from clotting by allowing the blood, as it escapes from the vessels, to run into a solution of sodium oxalate, using an amount such that the final mixture contains 0.1 per cent, of the oxalate. This mixture is cen- trifugalized, the clear plasma is removed, and the fibrinogen in it is precipitated by adding one part of a saturated solu- tion of sodium chloride. The precipitate is centrifugalized, the supernatant Liquid is decanted, and the precipitate further washed by the addition of a two-thirds saturated solution of *"Ardiiv f. Anat., Physiologie, u. wiss. Medicin," Eteichert u. du Bois- Reymond, L861, pp. 545, 675, and L862, pp. 428, 533; "Pfltlger's Archiv. f. d ge ammte Physiol., " 6, 413, L872; "Zur Blutlehre," Leipzig, L 892 and L895. f A number of the facts and points of view used in the following presenta lion of i lie ulijiTt of coii^iilni ion .urc new and are based upon experiments made in the author's laboratory by Dr. Rettger. "American Journal of Physiology," L909, 24, 406. COAGULATION. 443 sodium chloride. The precipitate is then dissolved in a solution of sodium chloride (2 per cent.), and is again precipitated and washed as above, and the process is repeated a third time. The final precipitate is dissolved in a 0.9 per cent, solution of sodium chloride and is then dialyzed for twelve hours in a collodium tube against a solution of 0.9 per cent, sodium' chloride to remove the last traces of the oxalate originally added to the blood. A solution of fibrinogen prepared as above clots readily upon the addition of blood-serum or of other solutions containing thrombin. As a matter of fact, such solutions may also clot, although much more slowly, when instead of a thrombin solu- tion one adds a little Ringer's mixture or a solution containing calcium chloride and sodium bicarbonate in about the proportion found in this mixture. This latter fact indicates that the fibrin- ogen solution as prepared contains some material from which thrombin may be produced in the presence of the calcium and the carbonate. Presumably this material is a prothrombin, and to remove it and obtain a solution which will not clot except upon the addition of ready formed thrombin one may make use of the fact that the prothrombin, like the thrombin, is readily carried down by precipitates. The solution of the fibrinogen may be wiped free of the prothrombin on forming in it a small precipitate of barium phosphate by the addition of barium chloride and sodium phosphate, this process being carried out before the final dialysis. Preparation of Thrombin. — Thrombin, or so-called fibrin ferment, is prepared most readily by the method first described by Schmidt. Blood is allowed to clot and the serum is then precipitated by the addition of a large excess of alcohol (usually twenty volumes). After standing some days or weeks the precipitate is drained off and dried, and is then ground up and extracted with water. The aqueous extract contains proteins, salts, and other things in addition to the thrombin. A solution made in this way causes a prompt coagulation when added to a solution of pure fibrinogen. That the thrombin thus obtained is not present as such in normal blood, but is formed after shedding, is indicated by the fact that if the animal's blood is allowed to flow directly from the artery into a large bulk of alcohol, due care being taken in the process, the precipitate thus obtained when subsequently dried and extracted, with water yields no thrombin. The Factors of Coagulation. — A pure solution of fibrinogen is converted to fibrin upon the addition of thrombin. These two substances constitute, therefore, the essential factors in the process of clotting. It has been shown, moreover, that cal- 444 BLOOD AND LYMPH. cium salts also play an essential part in the process. This fact was first demonstrated by the work of Arthus and Pages. If blood as it flows from the vessels is received into a solution of potassium or sodium oxalate, it may be kept indefinitely without clotting. The addition, however, at any moment of a small excess of calcium chloride will cause clotting to take place. This experiment, which any one may repeat, seems to show conclusively that calcium plays a specific role in the process of coagulation. Hammarsten has furnished the proof that the role of the calcium is connected with the formation of the thrombin and not apparently with the action of the thrombin on the fibrinogen. Thus he has shown that an oxalated solution of fibrinogen clots readily when some oxalated serum, containing ready formed thrombin, is added to it. In some way, which is not clearly understood, the calcium assists in the production of the thrombin from an antecedent substance which we may designate as a prothrombin or a thrombogen. The calcium salts may be regarded, therefore, as a necessary factor in coagulation, and it is possible that other organic or inorganic substances are concerned in the act of formation of the thrombin. It has long- been stated, for example, that extracts from various tissues of the body contain substances which facilitate the clotting of blood or other fibrinogen-containing solutions. Substances of this kind were designated by Schmidt as zymoplastic, and sub- sequent authors have applied different names to describe similar bodies — such as cytozym (Fuld-Spiro), coagulin (Loeb), throm- bokinase (Morawitz), etc. The specific action taken by these bodies has been differently interpreted by the several authors named, but it may perhaps lie doubted whether any conclusive proof has been furnished as vet for the existence of definite organic substances concerned in the preparation or activation of the thrombin. The theory proposed by Morawitz assumes that thrombokinase is a substance of the nature of an enzyme, which under normal conditions does not preexist in the blood, but is formed after shedding by the breaking down of the plates and leucocytes. With the assistance of the calcium salts this kinase converts Hie prosubstance (thrombogen) info the active thrombin. To repeat, therefore, we may say with some posi- tiveness thai the immediate factors of coagulation are fibrinogen and thrombin, and that in the production of the thrombin from prothrombin other factors are concerned, one of which at least is calcium in some form. Properties and Origin of Thrombin. Thrombin exists ready formed in fresh serum, a drop or so of such serum readily causing coagulation in solutions of fibrinogen. <)u standing, however, COAGULATION. 445 this property of the serum quickly diminishes and finally dis- appears, owing to the fact that the thrombin is either converted to another form (metathrombin) or, as seems more likely, is combined and rendered inactive by some substances in the serum. The serum in this condition can be reactivated readily by the addition of alkali or acid and subsequent neutralization, or it may be rendered active again by simply allowing it to stand until putrefaction has developed (Rettger). That the thrombin, once it is formed, is a stable body is shown by the fact that its aqueous solutions, prepared by Schmidt's method, do not lose their clotting power on boiling or on putrefaction. When fresh serum containing active thrombin is boiled or is heated to 65° C. the thrombin is apparently destroyed, but addition of alkali will again develop the power to cause the coagulation of fibrinogen solutions. Contrary to the usual statement made in the literature, it may be asserted, therefore, that heating serum does not destroy its contained thrombin, it simply causes its inactivation. It would seem that the processes of inactiva- tion which occur slowly at ordinary room temperatures are hastened greatly by the higher temperatures. In the older literature especially thrombin was spoken of as fibrin ferment, and it has been assumed usually that it belongs to the group of enzymes or ferments. Two facts, however, seem to make this view untenable. First, it is not destroyed when in solution by boiling, As stated above, aqueous solutions may be prepared by Schmidt's method, which after boiling still cause the formation of fibrin from fibrinogen. So also when serum containing thrombin is heated, the thrombin is not destroyed but inactivated. Second, in its action upon fibrinogen it does not conduct itself like a catalytic agent, which accelerates the conversion to fibrin without itself being used up in the process. On the contrary, it seems to form a definite combination with the fibrinogen, and the amount of fibrin formed varies with the amount of thrombin added up to a maximum limit. The resistance offered by thrombin to putre- faction would indicate that it may not be a protein body. It is nevertheless an organic colloidal substance, since it is not readily dialyzable, and it is destroyed when, in dry form, it is heated to the point of charring. As to the origin of the thrombin, all the evi- dence seems to point to the conclusion that it is formed directly from an antecedent substance which may be designated as pro- thrombin. It is, moreover, usually assumed by recent authors that the prothrombin exists preformed in the circulating blood. Thus it is stated that when oxalated plasma is cooled by ice a precipitate is obtained which when dissolved has itself no action upon fibrinogen solutions, but when calcium salts are added 446 BLOOD AND LYMPH. becomes converted to an effective thrombin. This and similar experiments, however, do not prove that the prothrombin is not formed after shedding from the breaking down of some of the corpuscular elements. Fresh blood caught under alcohol, with precautions against exposure, yields no thrombin and, moreover, gives no indication of the existence of prothrombin. When the aqueous extract of the precipitate is treated with acids, alkalies, or putrefaction, no thrombin is formed. We may fairly conclude that no prothrombin is thrown down by the alcohol and that, therefore, none is present in the circulating blood. Accepting this conclusion, we must believe that the prothrombin and thrombin are both formed after the blood is shed, and we can scarcely imagine any other origin for them than the corpuscular elements which disintegrate (blood-plates and leucocytes) after the blood leaves the blood-vessels. Micro- scopic evidence shows that the leucocytes and the agglutinated masses of blood-plates serve as points of origin for the formation of the fibrin threads, and we may suppose that these elements in the act of disintegrating, or by some process of secretion, give rise to the prothrombin. This latter substance is then converted to thrombin by the action of substances present in the plasma, the calcium salts being one at least of the factors that enter into this process of activation. Summary. — For the sake of clearness it may be said that perhaps the prevalent theory of coagulation at the present time is that advanced by Morawitz. The peculiarity of his view is the supposition that a kinase (thrombokinase) is liberated from cellular elements in the blood after shedding, and it is this kinase which with the cooperation of the calcium salts converts the prothrombin (thrombogen) into active thrombin. The theory may be expressed in diagrammatic form as follows: Cellular elements ©-► thrombokinase. Thrombokinase + calcium salts + thrombogen = thrombin. Thrombin + fibrinogen = fibrin. All the factors concerned are supposed to be present in the plasma of the circulating blood, with the exception of the throm- bokinase. The theory is apparently based upon an analogy with the action of enterokinase in converting trypainogen to trypsin, and it assumes, moreover, • that both the thrombin and the kinase are ferment bodies. As stated above, the facts known at present in regard to thrombin speak strongly against the view that it belongs to the group of enzymes, and so far as the exist- ence of a kinase: is concerned, the experimental work, upon mam- malian blood at Least, dues no1 furnish any necessary reason for adding this factor. Moreover, it seems very doubtful whether COAGULATION. 447 the prothrombin as such exists in the circulating blood. Bearing these points in mind, a more conservative statement of the steps taken in the process of coagulation would be the following: Corpuscular elements (plates, leucocytes) s-» prothrombin. Prothrombin + calcium salts (and other possible conditions) = thrombin. Thrombin + fibrinogen = fibrin. Other theories have been advanced in recent years, some of which may be referred to here very briefly, although for an adequate statement of details the originals must be consulted. Nolf assumes the existence of three or four fibrin factors — fibrinogen, hepatothrombin, leucothrombin, and calcium salts. The leucothrombin is a ferment body formed by leucocytes, although it may be produced also by the endothelial cells (yasothrombin). The fibrinogen and hepatothrombin are formed by the liver and are present con- stantly in the blood. In coagulation the hepatothrombin and leucothrombin unite to form an addition product, which is thrombin. The thrombin then combines physically with the fibrinogen, as one colloid may act upon another, with the production of fibrin. Coagulation is not a ferment action, and its normal ending is the digestion of the fibrin (fibrinolysis) by the proteolytic action of the leucothrombin. (See Nolf, "Arch, internationales de physiologie, " 1906, iv., 165 and 1908, vi., 1 and 115.) Loeb describes the presence of coagulins in the tissues which are different from the thrombin of blood, but in the presence of calcium salts are capable of acting directly on the fibrinogen and converting it to fibrin. (See "Bioch. Centralbl.," 1907, 829 and 899, Review of Literature.) Mellanby uses in general the nomen- clature of Morawitz, but assumes that prothrombin is always found associated with fibrinogen, so that kinase and solutions of calcium salts added to a fibrin- ogen solution will always suffice to cause coagulation, since the elements for the formation of the thrombin are present. (See "Journal of Physiology," 1908, 38, 28.) The Conversion of Fibrinogen to Fibrin. — The nature of the reaction between fibrinogen and thrombin is not at all under- stood. It was formerly supposed, on the basis of experiments made by Hammarsten, that the thrombin caused the fibrinogen molecule to split, the larger fraction forming the insoluble fibrin, while a smaller portion remained in solution as a globulin, the fibrin globulin. This view would be in accord with the prevalent assumption that the thrombin acts as a ferment, causing, prob- ably, from this standpoint, hydrolytic cleavage of the fibrinogen, after the manner of the digestive ferments. Later experiments have failed to confirm this view, and it has been suggested as another possibility that the thrombin causes in some way a molecular rearrangement of the fibrinogen, but this also is an hypothesis for which there is no experimental evidence. One of the most significent facts in this connection is the old obser- vation (Buchanan, Gamger) that the washed clot can be made to yield a thrombin by treatment with a solution of sodium chloride. Indeed, very carefully washed fibrin made from pure fibrinogen, which upon extraction with water yields no thrombin, may subsequently show a large amount if allowed to undergo 448 BLOOD AND LYMPH. decomposition by putrefaction (Rettger). Evidently then the thrombin does not undergo an essential chemical change in its reaction with fibrinogen, and one ma}' suppose, therefore, with Nolf, that its union with fibrinogen is physical or physicochemical rather than chemical. This supposition is supported, moreover, by the fact (Mellanby, Rettger) that the reaction is practically not affected, as to its velocity, by temperatures varying between 17° and 38° C, whereas, if the process was of the nature of an ordinary chemical reaction, we should expect, in accordance with van't Hoff's law, that the velocity would be doubled for an increase of 10° C. in temperature. Why Blood Does Not Clot Within the Blood-vessels — An- tithrombin. — The reason that blood remains fluid within the blood- vessels and coagulates in a few minutes after being shed would seem to be contained in the theories of coagulation just described. We may assume that in the living blood-vessels the formed elements — leucocytes and blood plates — do not disintegrate in great numbers at a time, and therefore do not give rise to any noticeable quantity of the antecedents of thrombin. It seems most probable that little, if any. thrombin is actually present in the blood under normal circumstances, and this in itself may be regarded as the main reason for the fact that the blood remains unclotted. It is quite possible, however, that other safeguards may exist in a matter of such prime importance. It has been shown, for instance, that, when solutions of fibrin ferment (thrombin) are injected into the circu- lation, clotting is not produced with the "certainty that one might expect. Delezenne has described experiments which indicate that the liver exercises a defensive power in this respect.* He states that when blood-serum — containing, as it normally does, some active thrombin — is circulated through a living liver it loses its power of inducing coagulation in solutions containing fibrinogen. [ts thrombin has been destroyed or made inactive by some effect of the liver, and it is possible, although not demonstrated as yet, that the liver may exercise such a protective action under special circumstances during life. That this supposed action of the liver is i H.t always essential is shown by the fact that in animals from whom the liver has been removed experimentally the blood does not clot within the vessels. The older observers were impressed with the fact thai blood remains u'ncoagulatcd for long periods if kepi m contacl with what may be culled its normal surface, — that is. the interior of the hearl or blood-vessels. In an excised heart or blood-vessel the blood, although a1 rest, remains fluid for a long time. H was thought possible, therefore, that the normal en- dothelial walls of the vessels exercise a restraining influence of * "Travaux ' 100. r The average results obtained from numerous experiments are as follows: The ratio of weight of blood to weight of body is, in the * "Zeitechrift f. physiol. Chemie," 31, 235. 1900. REGENERATION AFTER HEMORRHAGE. 453 dog, 7.7 per cent.; rabbit and cat, 5 per cent.; birds, 10 per cent. On man we have upon record two determinations on guillotined criminals made by Bischoff, which gave 7.7 and 7.2 per cent. Haldane and Smith,* however, have devised a modification of Grehant's carbon monoxid method, which they have applied to living men. The results of some 74 experiments gave them an average value of only 5 per cent, for man. The distribution of this blood in the tissues of the body at any time has been esti- mated by Ranke,f from experiments on freshly killed rabbits, as follows : Spleen 0.23 per cent. Brain and cord 1.24 " " Kidneys 1.63 " " Skin 2.10 " " Intestines 6.30 " " Bones 8.24 " " Heart, lungs, and great blood-vessels 22.76 " " Resting muscles 29.20 " " Liver 29.30 " " It will be seen from inspection of this table that in the rabbit the blood of the body is distributed at any one time about as follows: One-fourth to the heart, lungs, and great blood-vessels; one-fourth to the liver; one-fourth to the resting muscles; and one-fourth to the remaining organs. Regeneration of the Blood after Hemorrhage. — A large portion of the entire quantity of blood in the body may be lost suddenly by hemorrhage without producing a fatal result. The extent of hemorrhage that may be recovered from safely has been investigated upon a number of animals. Although the results show more or less individual variation, it may be said that in dogs a hemorrhage of from 2 to 3 per cent, of the body-weight t is re- covered from easily, while a loss of 4.5 per cent., more than half the entire blood, will probably prove fatal. In cats a hemorrhage of from 2 to 3 per cent, of the body-weight is not usually followed by a fatal result. Just what percentage of loss may be borne by the human being has not been determined, but it is probable that a healthy individual may recover without serious difficulty from the loss of a quantity of blood amounting to as much as 3 per cent, of the body-weight. It is known that if liquids that are isotonic to the blood, such as physiological saline (NaCl, 0.7 to 0.9 per cent.) * Haldane and Smith, "Journal of Physiology," 1900, xxv., 331; also Zuntz and Pletsch, "Biochemische Zeitschrif t, " 1908, 47. t Taken from Vierordt's "Anatomische, physiologische, und physikalische Daten und Tabellen," Jena, 1893. t Fredericq, "Travaux du Laboratoire" (Universite de Liege), 1, 189, 1885. 454 BLOOD AND LYMPH. or Ringer's solution, are injected into the veins immediately after a severe hemorrhage, recovery is more certain; in fact, it is possible by this means to restore persons after a hemorrhage that would otherwise have been fatal. By an infusion of this kind, particularly if at or somewhat above the body temperature, the heart beat is increased, the volume of the circulating liquid is brought to an amount sufficient to maintain approximately normal conditions of pressure and velocity, and the red corpuscles that still remain are kept in more rapid circulation and are thus utilized more completely as oxygen carriers. If a hemorrhage has not been fatal, experiments on lower animals show that the plasma of the blood is regenerated with some rapidity, the blood regaining its normal vol- 12 3 4 1 • •— ,=B" ^ \ ^7 \ A "^N^^" vt -,/ — - ~7 \R -Jt- ■ i - ^ ^%7 -, " \ / — , ^-3" X zv hv i \ 1 / V""^-^-^ ■""^ I / _ 100 Erythrocytes. 8,000,000 7,000,000(73%) 6,000,000(06%) 6,000,000 (55 %) 4,000,000(13%) (35%) 35,000(25%) 26,000(15%) !".■■■. Fig. 1*2. — To show the effect of hemorrhage upon the number of red and white cor- puscles and the amount of hemoglobin.- (Dawson.) The ordinates express tlie numbers of corpuscles and also the percentages of hemoglobin as stated in the figures to the left. 'I be ab ci a give the 'lavs after hemorrhage. The experiment was made upon a dope of fc.1 kgms. The hemorrhage, which lasted 2.3 minutes, was equal to 4.3 per cent, of the body-weight. An equal amount of physiological saline (NaCl, 0.8 per cent.) was injected immediately. lime within a few hours in slight hemorrhages, and in from twenty- four to forty-eight hours if the loss of blood has been severe; but the number of red corpuscles and the hemoglobin are regenerated more slowly, getting back to normal only after a number of days or after several weeks. The accompanying curves illustrate the results of a seven- hemorrhage ( L3 per cent, of the body-weight) followed by transfusion of an equal volume of physiological saline. So far as the rod corpuscles and (he amount of hemoglobin are concerned, BLOOD-TRANSFUSION. 455 it will be noticed that the large sudden fall from the hemorrhage, first day, is followed by a slower drop in both factors during the second and third days. This latter phenomenon constitutes what is known as the posthemorrhagic fall.* Blood-transfusion. — Shortly after the discovery of the circu- lation of the blood (Harvey, 1628), the operation was introduced of transfusing blood from one individual to another or from some of the lower animals to man. Extravagant hopes were held as to the value of such transfusion not only as a means of replacing the blood lost by hemorrhage, but also as a cure for various infirmities and diseases. Then and subsequently fatal as well as successful results followed the operation. It is now known to be a dangerous undertaking, mainly for two reasons: first, the strange blood, whether transfused directly or after defibrination, is liable to contain a quantity of thrombin sufficient, perhaps, to cause intravas- cular clotting; second, the serum of one animal may be toxic to another or cause a destruction of its blood corpuscles. Owing to this hemolytic and toxic action, which has previously been referred to (p. 414), the injection of foreign blood is likely to be directly injurious instead of beneficial. In cases of loss of blood from severe hemorrhage, therefore, it is far safer to inject a neutral liquid, such as the so-called "physiological salt solution" — a solution of sodium chlorid of such a strength (0.7 to 0.9) as will suffice to prevent hemolysis of the red corpuscles. * Dawson, "American Journal of Physiology," 4, 1, 1900. CHAPTER XXIV. COMPOSITION AND FORMATION OF LYMPH. Lymph is a colorless liquid found in the lymph-vessels as well as in the extra vascular spaces of the body. All the tissue elements, in fact, may be regarded as being bathed in lymph. To understand its occurrence in the body one has only to bear in mind its method of origin from the blood. Throughout the entire body there is a rich supply of blood-vessels penetrating every tissue with the ex- ception of the epidermis and some epidermal structures, as the nails and the hair. The plasma of the blood, by the action of physical or chemical processes, the details of which are not yet entirely under- stood, makes its way through the thin walls of the capillaries, and is thus brought into immediate contact with the tissues, to which it brings the nourishment and oxygen of the blood and from which it removes the waste products of metabolism. This extra vascular lymph is collected into small capillary spaces which in turn open into definite lymphatic Vessels. It is still a question among the his- tologists whether the lymph- vessels form a closed system or are in direct anatomical connection with the tissue spaces. Modern work* supports the view that the lymph capillaries are closed vessels similar in structure to the blood capillaries. They end in the tissues generally, but are not in open communication with the spaces between the cellular elements or with the larger serous cavities between the folds of the peritoneum, pleura, etc., or with the spaces between the meningeal membranes surrounding the central nervous system. From the physiological standpoint, however, the liquid in these latter cavities, the cerebrospinal liquid and the liquid bathing the tissue elements, must be regarded as a part of the general supply of Lymph and as being in communication with the liquid contained in the lymph- els. That is to say, the water and the dissolved substances contained in the tissue spaces interchange more or less freely with the Lymph proper found in the formed Lymph-vessels. The lymph-vessels unite to form Larger and larger trunks, making eventually one main trunk, the thoracic or lclt lymphatic duct, * See Babin, "American Journal of Anatomy," I, '-'><'>7, L902, and :'>, is:',, 1904; .il o "General and Special Anatomy of the Lymphatics," from Poiriei and Charpy, fcran lated l>y Leaf, L904. 156 COMPOSITION AND FORMATION OF LYMPH. 457 and a second smaller right lymphatic duct, which open into the blood-vessels, each on its own side, at the junction of the sub- clavian and internal jugular veins. While the supply of lymph in the lymph-vessels may be considered as being derived ulti- mately entirely from the blood-plasma, it is well to bear in mind that at any given moment this supply may be altered by direct interchange with the plasma on one side and the extravascular lymph permeating the tissue elements on the other. The intravascular lymph may be augmented, for example, by a flow of water from the blood-plasma into the lymph-spaces, and thence into the lymph-vessels, or by a flow from the tissue elements into the lymph-spaces that surround them. The lymph movement is from the tissues to the veins, and the flow is main- tained chiefly by the difference in pressure between the lymph at its origin in the tissues and in the large lymphatic vessels. The continual formation of lymph in the tissues leads to the development of a relatively high pressure in the lymph capil- laries, and as a result of this the lymph is forced toward the point of lowest pressure — namely, the points of junction of the large lymph-ducts with the venous system. A brief discussion of the factors concerned in the movement of lymph will be found in the section on Circulation. As would be inferred from its origin, the composition of the intravascular lymph is essentially the same as that of blood-plasma. It contains the three blood proteins, the extractives (urea, fat, lecithin, cholesterin, sugar), and inorganic salts. The salts are found in the same proportions as in the plasma; the proteins are less in amount, especially the fibrinogen. Lymph coagulates, but does so more slowly and less firmly than the blood. Histologically, lymph consists of a colorless liquid containing a number of leucocytes, and after meals a number of minute fat droplets; red blood corpuscles occur only accidentally, and blood-plates, according to most accounts, are likewise normally absent. The composition of the exudative liquids of the body, such as the pericardial liquid, the synovial liquid, the aqueous humor, the cerebrospinal liquid, etc., which are sometimes classed under the general term lymph, may vary greatly; thus, the cerebrospinal liquid possesses no morphological elements, contains no fibrinogen, and, therefore, does not clot, and, indeed, has only minute traces of protein of any kind. Formation of Lymph. — The careful researches of Ludwig and his pupils were formerly believed to prove that the lymph is derived directly from the plasma of the blood mainly by filtration through the capillary walls. Emphasis was laid on the undoubted fact that the blood within the capillaries is under a pressure higher than that 45S BLOOD AND LYMPH. prevailing in the tissues outside, and it was supposed that this excess of pressure is sufficient to squeeze the plasma of the blood through the very thin capillary walls. Various conditions that alter the pressure of the blood were shown to influence the amount of lymph formed in accordance with the demands of a theory of filtration. Moreover, the composition of lymph as usually given seems to sup- port such a theory, inasmuch as the inorganic salts contained in it are in the same concentration, approximately, as in blood-plasma, while the proteins are in less concentration, following the well- known law that in the filtration of colloids through animal mem- branes the filtrate is more dilute than the original solution. This simple and apparently satisfactory theory has been subjected to critical examination within recent years, and it has been shown that filtration alone does not suffice to explain the composition of the lymph under all circumstances. At present two divergent views are held upon the subject. According to some physiologists, all the facts known with regard to the composition of lymph may be satisfactorily explained if we suj3pose that this liquid is formed from blood-plasma by the combined action of the physical processes of filtration, diffusion, and osmosis. According to others, it is believed that, in addition to filtration and diffusion, it is necessary to assume an active secretory process on the part of the endothe- lial cells composing the capillary walls. The actual condition of our knowledge of the subject can be presented most easily by briefly stating some of the objections that have been raised by Heidenhain* to a pure filtration-and-diffusion theory, and indicating how these objections have been met. 1. Heidenhain showed by simple calculations that an impossible formation of lymph would be required, upon the filtration theory, to supply the chemical needs of the organs in various organic and in- organic constituents. Thus, to take an illustration that has been much discussed, one kilogram of cows' milk contains 1.7 gms. CaO and the entire milk of twenty-four hours would contain, in round numbers, 42.5 gms, CaO. Since the lymph contains normally about 0.18 part of CaO per thousand, it would require 2o6 liters of lymph per day to supply the necessary Ca< ) to the mammary glands. Heidenhain himself suggests that the difficulty in this case may be met by assuming active diffusion processes in connection with filtration. If, for instance, in the case cited, we suppose that the calcium of the lymph is quickly combined by the tissues of the mam- mary gland, then the tension of calcium salts in the lymph will be kept nt zero, and an active diffusion of calcium into the lymph will occur so long as the gland is secreting. En other words, the gland will receh e its calcium by much the same process as it receives its * "Archivf. dicgesammte Physiologic," 49, 209, 1891. COMPOSITION AND FORMATION OF LYMPH. 459 oxygen, and will get its daily supply from a comparatively small bulk of lymph. Strictly speaking, therefore, the difficulty we are dealing with here shows only the insufficiency of a pure filtration theory. It seems possible that filtration and diffusion together would suffice to supply the organs, so far at least as the diffusible substances are concerned. 2. Heidenhain found that occlusion of the inferior vena cava causes not only an increase in the flow of lymph — as might be ex- pected, on the filtration theory, from the consequent rise of pressure in the capillary regions — but also an increased concentration in the percentage of protein in the lymph. This latter fact has been satisfactorily explained by the experiments of Starling.* Accord- ing to this observer, the lymph formed in the liver is normally more concentrated than that of the rest of the body. The occlusion of the vena cava causes a marked rise in the capillary pressure in the liver, and most of the increased lymph-flow under these circum- stances comes from the liver; hence the greater concentration. The results of this experiment, therefore, do not antagonize the filtration-and-diffusion theory. 3. Heidenhain discovered that extracts of various substances, which he designated as " lymphagogues of the first class," cause a marked increase in the flow of lymph from the thoracic duct, the lymph being more concentrated than normal, and the increased flow continuing for a long period. Nevertheless, these substances cause little, if any, increase in general arterial pressure; in fact, if injected in sufficient quantity they produce usually a fall of arterial pressure. The substances belonging to this class comprise such things as pep- tone, egg-albumin, extracts of liver and intestine, and especially extracts of the muscles of crabs, crayfish, mussels, and leeches. Heidenhain supposed that these extracts contain an organic substance which acts as a specific stimulus to the endothelial cells of the capillaries and increases their secretory action. The results of the action of these substances has been differently explained by those who are unwilling to believe in the secretion theory. Starling f finds experimentally that the increased flow of lymph in this case, as after obstruction of the vena cava, comes mainly from the liver. There is at the same time in the portal area an increased pressure that may account in part for the greater flow of lymph ; but, since this effect upon the portal pressure lasts but a, short time, while the greater flow of lymph may continue for one or two hours, it is obvious that this factor alone does not suffice to explain the result of the injections. Starling suggests, therefore, that these extracts act pathologically upon the blood capillaries, particularly those of * "Journal of Physiology," 16, 234, 1S!»4. t Ibid., 17, 30, 1894. 460 BLOOD AND LYMPH. the liver, and render them more permeable, so that a greater quantity of concentrated lymph flows through them. Starling's explanation is supported by the experiments of Popoff.* According to this observer, if the lymph is collected simultaneously from the lower portion of the thoracic duct, which conveys the lymph from the abdominal organs, and from the upper part, which contains the lymph from the head, neck, etc., it is found that injection of peptone increases the flow only from the abdominal organs. Popoff finds also that the peptone causes a dilatation in the intestinal circulation and a marked rise in the portal pressure. At the same time there is some evidence of injury to the walls of the blood- vessels from the occurrence of extravasations in the intestine. As far, therefore, as the action of the lymphagogues of the first class is concerned, it may be said that the advocates of the filtration-and- diffusion theory have suggested a plausible explanation in accord with their theory. The facts emphasized by Heidenhain with regard to this class of substances do not compel us to assume a secretory function for the endothelial cells. 4. Injection of certain crystalline substances — such as sugar, sodium chlorid, and other neutral salts — causes a marked increase in the flow of lymph from the thoracic duct. The lymph in these cases is more dilute than normal, and the blood-plasma also becomes more watery, thus indicating that the increase in water comes from the tissues themselves. Heidenhain designated these bodies as "lymphagogues of the second class." His explanation of their action is that the crystalloid materials introduced into the blood are eliminated by the secretory activity of the endothelial cells, and that they then attract water from the tissue liquid, thus augmenting the flow of lymph. These substances cause but little change in arterial blood-pressure ; hence Heidenhain thought that the greater flow of lymph can not be explained by an increased filtration. Starling f has shown, however, that, although these bodies may not seriously alter general arterial pressure, they may greatly augment intracapillary pressure, particularly in the abdominal organs. His explanation of the greater flow of lymph in these cases is as follows: " ( )n their injection into the blood the osmotic pressure of the circu- lating fluid is largely increased. In consequence of this increase water is attracted from lymph and tissues into the blood by a process of osmosis, until the osmotic pressure of the circulating fluid is restored to normal. A condition of hydremic plethora is thereby produced, attended with a rise of pressure in the capillaries generally, especially in those of the abdominal viscera. This rise of pressure will be proportional t" I Ik- increase in the volumeof the blood, and * "Ccntralblatt f. Physiologic," \), No. 2, L895. f hoc. cit. COMPOSITION AND FORMATION OF LYMPH. 461 therefore to the osmotic pressure of the solutions injected. The rise of capillary pressure causes great increase in the transudation of fluid from the capillaries, and therefore in the lymph-flow from the thoracic duct." This explanation is well supported by experi- ments, and seems to obviate the necessity of assuming a secretory action on the part of the capillary walls. 5. Numerous otherexperiments have been devised by Heidenhain and his followers to show that the physical laws of filtration, diffu- sion, and osmosis do not suffice to explain the production of lymph ; but in all these cases possible explanations have been suggested in terms of the physical laws, so that it may be said that the facts do not compel us to assume a secretory activity on the part of the endothelial cells of the capillaries. Asher* and his co-workers have brought forward many facts to show that the lymph is controlled as to its amount by the activity of the tissue elements and may be considered as a product of the activity of the tissues, as a secretion, in fact, of the working cells. When the salivary glands, the liver, etc., are in greater functional activity the flow of lymph from them is increased beyond doubt, so that the activity of the organs does influence most markedly the production of lymph. Most physiologists, however, prefer to explain this relationship on the view suggested by Koranyi, Starling, and others, — namely, that in the metabolic changes of functional activity the large molecules of protein, fat, etc., are broken down to a number of simpler ones, the number of particles in solution is increased and therefore the osmotic pressure is increased. According to most observers the molecular concentration of the lymph in the thoracic duct, and, therefore, the osmotic pressure, is greater than that of the blood. Thus Botazzi,f in one experiment, reports that the lowering of the freezing-point of the blood-serum was A =0.595° C, while that of the lymph from the thoracic duct of the same animal was A =0.615° C. Back in the tissues, where phys- iological oxidations are going on, this difference is probably greater, and greater in proportion to the activity of the tissues. We can understand that in this way functional activity of an organ may result in attracting more water from the blood-capil- laries into the tissue spaces and may thus cause an augmented flow of lymph. The liquid of the tissues may be drained off not only through the lymph-vessels but also through the blood- vessels. That liquids injected directly into the tissues or special substances dissolved in such liquids may be absorbed directly by the blood has long been known. Magendie, for example, * "Zeitschrift f. Biologie," vols, xxxvi-xl. 1S97 to 1900. f Quoted from Magnus, "Handbuch der Biochemie, " 1908, vol. ii.2 (Formation of Lymph) . 462 - BLOOD AND LYMPH. proved that when a poison was injected into an organ which was connected with the rest of the body only by its blood-vessels, the animal quickly showed the symptoms of a corresponding intoxication. Ordinary hypodermic injections are absorbed much more quickly into the general circulation than would-be the case if they were obliged to traverse the lymph-vessels and enter the blood through the thoracic duct. Meltzer has shown that this absorption by the blood from the tissue spaces takes place with especial promptness if the injection is made into a mass of muscular tissue. The liquid in the extravascular tissue spaces is, in fact, sub- ject to a play of influences from several sides, and it is the bal- ance among these competing influences which determines at any time the amount and composition of this tissue lymph. Thus, the supply of this liquid is furnished, on the one hand, by water and dissolved substances coming to it from the blood in the capillaries, on the other hand, by water and dissolved substances derived from the great reservoir contained in the tissue cells. The amount of the tissue lymph is continually depleted on the other side by water and dissolved substances passing back into the capillaries, or into the tissue elements, or, finally, into the lymph capillaries. The amount that passes by this latter route varies greatly in the different tissues, and in the same tissue may be influenced greatly by pathological as well as normal changes in conditions. Summary of the Factors Controlling the Flow of Lymph. — We may adopt, provisionally at least, the so-called mechanical theory of the origin of lymph. Upon this theory the forces in activity are, first, the intracapillary pressure tending to filter the plasma through the endothelial cells composing the walls of the capillaries; second, the force of diffusion depending upon the inequality in chemical composition of the blood-plasma and the liquid outside the capillaries, or, on the other side, between this latter liquid and the contents of the tissue elements; third, the force of osmotic pressure, which varies with the molecular concentration. These three forces acting everywhere control primarily the amount and composition of the lymph; but still another factor must be considered; for when we come to examine the flow of lymph in different parts of the body striking differ- ences are found. It has been shown, for instance, that in the limbs, under normal conditions, the flow is extremely scanty, while from the liver and the intestinal area it is relatively abundant. In fact, the lymph of the thoracic duct may be considered as being derived almost entirely from the latter two regions. Moreover, the lymph from the liver is characterized by COMPOSITION AND FORMATION OF LYMPH. 463 a greater percentage of proteins. To account for these differences Starling suggests the plausible explanation of a variation in permea- bility in the capillary walls. The capillaries seem to have a similar structure all over the body so far as this is revealed to us by the microscope, but the fact that the lymph-flow varies so much in quantity and composition indicates that the similarity is only superficial, and that in different organs the capillary walls may have different internal structures, and therefore different permea- bilities. This factor is evidently one of great importance. The idea that the permeability of the capillaries may vary under dif- ferent conditions has long been used in pathology to explain the production of that excess of lymph which gives rise to the condition of dropsy or edema. The theories and experiments made in con- nection with this pathological condition have, in fact, a direct bearing upon the theories of lymph formation.* Under normal conditions the lymph is drained off as it is formed, while under pathological conditions it may accumulate in the tissues owing either to an excessive formation of lymph or to some interruption in its circu- lation. The scanty flow of lymph from the limbs has been referred by Magnusf to another possible cause, namely, to the great capacity of the muscular tissue to imbibe water (and salts). According to this author the tissues, particularly the muscular tissues, constitute great reservoirs in which excess of water and salts may be stored. If, for example, a hypotonic solution of sodium chloride is injected into the circulation, most of the water added will be removed from the circulation by imbibition into the muscular tissues. In the limbs, with their large supply of muscular tissue, it may be that lymph is formed as elsewhere from the blood plasma, but it is held back from the lymph-vessels by absorption into the muscular mass. From the foregoing considerations it is evident that changes in capillary pressure, however produced, may alter the flow of lymph from the blood-vessels to the tissues, by increasing or decreasing, as the case may be, the amount of filtration; changes in the composition of the blood, such as follow periods of digestion, will cause diffusion and osmotic streams tending to equalize the composition of blood and lymph; and changes in the tissues them- selves following upon physiological or pathological activity will also disturb the equilibrium of "composition, and, therefore, set up diffusion and osmotic currents. In this way a continual interchange is taking place by means of which the nutrition of the tissues is * Consult Meltzer, "Edema" ("Harrington Lectures"), "American Medicine, " 8, Nos. 1, 2, 4.. and 5, 1904. f Magnus, Loc cit. 464 BLOOD AND LYMPH. effected, each according to its needs. The details of this interchange must of necessity be very complex when we consider the possibilities of local effects in different parts of the body. The total effects of general changes, such as may be produced experimentally, are simpler, and, as we have seen, are explained satisfactorily by the physical and chemical factors enumerated. SECTION V. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE ORGANS OF CIRCULA- TION OF THE BLOOD AND LYMPH. The heart and the blood-vessels form a closed vascular system containing a certain amount of blood. This blood is kept in endless circulation mainly by the force of the muscular contractions of the heart. But the bed through which it flows varies greatly in width at different parts of the circuit, and the resistance offered to the moving blood is very much greater in the capillaries than in the large vessels. It follows from the irregularities in size of the chan- nels through which it flows that the blood-stream is not uniform in character throughout the entire circuit; indeed, just the opposite is true. From point to point in the branching system of vessels the blood varies in regard to its velocity, its head of pressure, etc. These variations are connected in part with the fixed structure of the system and in part are dependent upon the changing properties of the living matter of which the system is composed. It is con- venient to consider the subject under three general heads: (1) The purely physical factors of the circulation, — that is, the me- chanics and hydrodynamics of the flow of a definite quantity of blood through a set of fixed tubes of varying caliber under certain fixed conditions. (2) The general physiology of the heart and the blood-vessels, — that is, mainly the special properties of the heart muscle and the plain muscle of the blood-vessels. (3) The innerva- tion of the heart and the blood-vessels, — that is, the variations in the circulation produced by the action of the nervous system. CHAPTER XXV. THE VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF THE BLOOD-FLOW. The Circulation as Seen Under the Microscope. — It is a comparatively easy matter to arrange a thin membrane in a living animal so that the flowing blood may be observed with the aid of a microscope. For such a purpose one generally employs the web between the toes of a frog, or better still the mesentery, lungs, or 30 465 466 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. bladder of the same animal. With a good preparation many important peculiarities in the blood-flow may be observed directly. If the field is properly chosen one may see at the same time the flow in arteries, capillaries, and veins. It will be noticed that in the arteries the flow is very rapid and somewhat intermittent, — that is, there is a slight acceleration of velocity, a pulse, with each heart beat. In the capillaries, on the contrary, the flow is relatively very slow; the change from the rushing arterial stream to the deliber- ate current in the capillaries takes place, indeed, with some suddenness. The capillary flow, as a rule, shows no pulses corre- sponding with the heart beats, but it may be more or less irregular, — that is, the flow may nearly cease at times in some capillaries, while again it maintains a constant flow. In the veins the flow increases markedly in rapidity, and indeed it may be observed that, the larger the vein, the more rapid is the flow. There is not, however, as a rule, any indication of an intermittence or pulse in this flow, — the velocity is entirely uniform. In both arteries and veins it will be noticed that the red corpuscles form a solid column or core in the middle of the vessel, and that between them and the inner wall there is a layer of plasma containing only, under normal conditions, an occasional leucocyte. The accumulation of cor- puscles in the middle of the stream makes what is known as the axial stream, while the clear layer of plasma is designated as the inert layer. The phenomenon is readily explained by physical causes. As the blood flows rapidly through the small vessels the layers nearer the wall are slowed by adhesion, so that the greatest velocity is attained in the middle or axis of the vessel. The cor- puscles, being heavier than the plasma, are drawn into this rapid part of the current. It has been shown by physical experiments that, when particles of different specific gravities are present in a liquid flowing rapidly through tubes, the heavier particles will be found in the axis and the lighter ones toward the periphery. In accordance with this fact, leucocytes, which are lighter than the red corpuscles, may be found in the inert layer. When the con- ditions become slightly abnormal (incipient inflammation) the leucocytes increase in number in the inert layer sometimes to a very great extent, owing apparently to some alteration in the endothelial walls whereby the leucocytes are rendered more ad- hesive. The agglutination of the leucocytes and their migration through the walls into the surrounding tissues are described in works on Pathology. The Velocity of the Blood-flow. — The microscopical observa- tions described above show that the velocity of the blood-current varies widely, beinfj; rapid in the arteries and veins and slow in the capillaries. To ascertain the actual velocity in the larger vessels and the variations in vessels of different sizes experimental de- VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 467 terminations are necessary. While the general principle involved in these determinations is simple, their actual execution in an experiment is attended with some difficulties, and various devices have been adopted. The most direct method per- haps is that usqd in the in-' strument devised by Ludwig, — ■ namely, the stromuhr. The prin- ciple used is to cut an artery or vein of a known size and de- termine how much blood flows out in a given time. We may define the velocity of the blood at any point as the length of the column of blood flowing by that point in a second. If we cut the artery there a cylindri- cal column of blood of a defi- nite length and with a cross-area equal to that of the lumen of the artery will flow out in a second. The volume of the outflow can be determined di- rectly by catching the blood. Knowing this volume and the ™ Kg. 183.-— Ludwig's stromuhr: a and b, . . I he glass bulbs; a is filled with oil to the CrOSS-area Of the artery, we Can mark (5 c.c), while b and the neck are filled i , • ,i t .-1 » ,, with salt solution or defibrinated blood: p, determine the length Of the the movable plate by means of which the bulbs may be turned through 180 degrees, c, c, for the cannulas inserted into the artery; s. the thumb screw for turning the bulbs; h, the holder. When in place the clamps on the arteries are removed, blood flows through c into a, driving out the oil and column — that is, the velocity of the flow — since in a cylinder the volume, V, is equal to the product Of the length into the forcing the salt solution m b into the head end oi the artery through c . When the cross-area. V = length X cross-area, or V length = cross-area blood entering a reaches the mark, the bulbf are turned through 180 degrees so that b lies over c. The blood flows into b and drives the oil back into a. When it just fills this bulb, they are again rotated through 180 degrees, and so on. The oil is driven out of and into a a given number of times, each movement being equal to an outflow of 5 c.c. of blood. When the instrument has been We Cannot, Of COUrse, make turned, say, ten .times, 50 c.c. of blood have 1 . ' .. ... flowed out. Knowing the time and the the experiment in thlS Simple caliber of the artery, the calculation is made v • , ., as described in the text. Several modify Way Upon a living animal ; the cations of the form of this instrument have loss of so much blood would at been devisecL once change the physical and physiological conditions of the circula- tion, and would give us a set of conditions at the end of the experi- *A modification by Tigerstedt is described in the "Skandinavisches Archiv f. Physiol.," 3, 152, 1891. One by Burton-Opitz in the "Arch. f. d. ges. Physiologie," 121, 151, 1908. 468 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. merit different from those at the beginning. By means of the stromuhr, however, this experiment can be made, with this important variation, that the blood that flows from the central end of the cut artery is returned to the peripheral end of the same artery, so that the circulation is not blocked nor deprived of its normal volume of liquid. The instrument, as is explained in the legend of Fig. 183, measures the volume of blood that flows out of the cut end of an artery in a definite time. The calculation for velocity is made as follows: Suppose that the capacity of the bulb is 5 c.c, and that in the experiment it has been filled 10 times in 50 seconds, — i. e., the bulbs have been reversed 10 times; then obviously 10 X 5 or 50 c.c. have flowed out of the artery in this time, or 1 c.c. in 1 second. The diameter of the vessel can be meas- ured, and if found equal, say, to 2 mms., then its cross-area is tt2 = 3.15 X 1 = 3.15. Since 1 c.c. equals 1000 c.mm., the length of our cyl- inder of blood would be given by the quotient of 3 15- = 317 mms. So that the blood in this case was moving with the velocity of 317 mms. per second. Another instru- ment that has been employed for the same purpose is the dromograph or hcmodromograph of Chauveau. This instrument is represented in the accompanying figure (Fig. 184). A rigid tube (p-c) is placed in the course of the artery to be examined. This tube is provided with an offset (a) the opening of which is closed with rubber dam (m). The rubber dam is pierced by a needle the lower end of which terminates in a small plate lying in the tube (pi). When the instrument is in place and the blood is allowed to stream through the tube, it deflects the needle, which turns on its insertion through the. rubber as a ful- r-rum. The anfde of defieM.ion of the free end of the needle may be measured directly upon a scale or it may be transmitted through tambours and recorded upon a kymographion. The in- strument must, of course, be graduated by passing through it cur- Fig. 184. — Chauveau's hemodromo- grapfa (after Lanyendorff). The tube, ■ laced in the course of an ar- tery, the blood after removal of clamps flowing in the direction shown by the arrow. The current strikes the plate, pi, and forces it to an angle varying With the velocity. The movement of pi i transmitted through the stem, «, which moves in a rubber membrane a- a fulcrum, >"■ The angular move- ment of the projecting end of ra may be measured directly or may be made to ad upon a tambour, as shown in the figure, and thus be transmitted to n recording di urn. VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 469 rents of known velocity, so that the angle of deflection may be expressed in terms of absolute velocities. It possesses the great advantage over the stromuhr that it gives not simply the average velocity during a given time, but also the variations in velocity coincident with the heart beat or other changes that may occur during the period of observation. Efforts have been made to devise a method for the determination of the velocity of the blood-flow in the arteries of man. The method used, however, depends upon certain assumptions that are not entirely certain and the re- sults obtained, therefore, can not be used with confidence. The principle of the method consists * in determining the volume of the arm by placing it in a plethysmograph. Assuming that the outflow from the veins is constant in the part of the arm inclosed, then the variations in volume of the arm may be referred to the greater inflow of blood into this part through the arteries. The curve showing the variations in volume may, therefore, under proper conditions, be interpreted in terms of velocity changes. Mean Velocity of the Blood-flow in the Arteries, Veins, and Capillaries. — Actual determinations of the average velocity in the large arteries and veins give such results as the following: Carotid of horse (Volkmann), 300 mms. per second; (Chauveau) 297 mms. Carotid of the dog (Vierordt), 260 mms. The flow in the carotid, as in the other large arteries, is not, however, uniform; there is a marked acceleration or pulse at each systole of the heart during which the velocity is greatly augmented. Thus, in the carotid of the horse it has been shown by the hemo- dromograph that during the systole the velocity may reach 520 mms. and may fall to 150 mms. during the diastole. It is found, also, that this difference between the systolic velocity and the diastolic velocity tends to disappear as the arteries become smaller, and, as was said above, disappears altogether in the capillaries, in which the pulse caused by the heart beat is lacking. The smaller the artery, therefore, the more uniform is the movement of the blood. The flow in the large veins is uniform or approximately uniform and increases as one approaches the heart, although the velocity in the large veins near the heart is somewhat slower than in the large arteries of the same region, owing to the fact that the total area of the venous bed is larger than that of the arterial bed. Burton-Opitzf gives the following average figures obtained from experiments upon anesthetized dogs. Jugular, 147 mms.; femoral, 61.6 mms.; renal, 63 mms.; mesenteric vein, 84.9 mms. In the capillaries, however, the velocity is relatively very small. From direct observations made by means of the microscope and from indirect observations in the case of man, the capillary velocity is estimated as lying between 0.5 mm. and 0.9 mm. per sec. * Von Kries, " Archiv f. Physiologie, " 1887, 279; also Abeles, ibid., 1892, 22. f Burton-Opitz, "Am. Journal of Physiology," vols. 7 and 9, and "Pfluger's Archiv," vols. 123 and 124, 1908. 470 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. Vierordt reports some interesting calculations upon the velocity of the blood, in the capillaries of his own eye. Under suitable conditions,* the movements of the corpuscles in the retina may be perceived in consequence of the shadows that they tlirow upon the rods and cones. The visual images thus produced may be projected upon a surface at a known distance from the eye and the space traversed in a given time may be observed. The distance actually covered upon the retina may then be calculated by the following con- struction, in which A-B = the distance traveled by the projected image; .4.-/2, the distance of the surface from the eye; and a-n, the distance of the retina from the nodal point of the eye. We have then the proportion ab : an :: AB X an. An AB : An, or ab = According to this method, Vierordt calculated that the velocity of the blood in the human capillaries is equal to about 0.6 to 0.9 mm. per second. In the arteries, more- , , . Fig. 185. — Diagram of the eve to show the con- OVer, It may be Observed struction used to determine the' size of the retinal 4-V.^i- 4-U„ _ _ i "j. image when the size of the external object is known: that the average Velocity n, The nodal point of the eye. See text. diminishes the farther one goes from the heart, — that is, the smaller the artery, — and reaches its minimum when the arteries pass into the capillaries. Thus, Volkmann reports for the horse the following figures: Ca- rotid, 300 mms.; maxillary, 232; metatarsal, 56 rams. In the veins also the same fact holds. The smaller the vein — that is, the nearer it is to the capillary region — the smaller is its velocity, the maxi- lig. 186. — Schematic representation of the relative velocities of the blood-current in >.i lie .1 « mI m . v torn: «, The arterial side, indicating the changes with each heart beat and the fall of mean velocity as the arterial bed widens; c, the capillary ill'- gieat diminution in veloeit y em re, pond- with the groat widening of the bed; r, the ' :' Showing the gradual increase Inward the heart, and represented as entirelj uniform, although, as a matter of fact, the velocity in the large veins is affected by the respirations and to a small extent by the heart beat, owing t<> the phenomenon known as the venous pulse p. 513). mum velocity being found in the vena cava. The general rela- tions of the velocity of the blood in the arteries, capillaries, and veins may be expressed, therefore, by a curve such as is shown in Fig. iso. * "Archiv I', physiologische Heilkunde, " 15, 2~>.r>, ix.r>(j. VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 471 Explanation of the Variations in Velocity. — The general rela- tionship between the velocities in the different parts of the vascular system is explained by the difference in the width of the bed in which the blood flows. In the systemic circulation the main stem, the aorta, branches into arteries which, taken individually, are smaller and smaller as we approach the capillaries. But each time that an artery branches the sum of the areas of the two branches is greater than that of the main stem. The arterial system may be compared, in fact, to a tree, the sum of the cross-areas of all the twigs is greater than that of the main trunk. It follows, there- fore, that the blood as it passes to the capillaries flows in a bed or is distributed in a bed which becomes wider and wider, and as it returns to the heart in the veins it is collected into a bed that be- comes smaller as we approach the heart. Vierorclt estimates that the combined calibers of all the capillaries in the systemic circula- tion would make a tube with a cross-area about 800 times as large as the aorta. If the circulation is proceeding uniformly it follows that for any given unit of time the same volume of blood must pass through any given cross-section of the system, — that is, at a given point in the aorta or vena cava as much blood must flow by in a second as passes through the capillary region — and that consequently where the cross-section or bed is widest the velocity is correspondingly diminished. If the capillary bed is 800 times that of the aorta, then the velocity in the capillaries is -g^-g- of that in the aorta, — say, -g-g-g- of 320 mms. or 0.4 mm. Just as a stream of water flowing under a constant head reaches its greatest velocity where its bed is narrowest and flows more slowly where the bed widens to the dimensions of a pool or lake. Variations in Velocity with Changes in the Heart-beat or the Size of the Vessels. — While the above statement holds true as an explanation of the general relationship between the velocities in the arteries, veins, and capillaries at any given moment, the absolute velocities in the different parts of the system will, of course, vary whenever any of the conditions acting upon the blood-flow vary. In the large arteries, as has been said, there are extreme fluctua- tions in velocity at each heart beat; but if we consider only the average velocities it may be said that these will vary throughout the system with the force and rate of the heart beat, or with the variations in size of the small arteries and the resulting changes in blood-pressure in the arteries. Marey* gives the two following laws: (1) Whatever increases or diminishes the force with which the blood is driven from the heart toward the periphery will cause the velocity of the blood and the pressure in the arteries to vary in the same sense. (2) Whatever increases or diminishes the resis- tance offered to the blood in passing from the arteries (to the veins) * "La Circulation du Sang," Paris, 1SS1, p. 321. 47'2 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. will cause the velocity and the arterial pressure to vary in an inverse sense as regards each other. That, is, an increased re- sistance diminishes the velocity in the arteries while increasing the pressure, and vice versa. The Time Necessary for a Complete Circulation of the Blood. — It is a matter of interest in connection with man}' physio- logical questions to have an approximate idea of the time necessary for the blood to make a complete circuit of the vascular system, — that is, starting from any one point to determine how long it will take for a particle of blood to arrive again at the same spot. In considering such a question it must be borne in mind that many different paths are open to the blood, and that the time for a complete circulation will vary somewhat with the circuit actually followed. For example, blood leaving the left ventricle may pass through the coronary system to the right heart and thence through the pulmonary system to the left heart again, or it may pass to the extremities of the toes before getting to the right heart, or it may pass through the intestines, in which case it will have to traverse three capillary areas before completing the circuit. It is obvious, there- fore, that any figures obtained can only be regarded as averages more or less exact. The experiments that have been made, however, are valuable in indicating how very rapidly any substance that enters the blood may be distributed over the body. The method first employed by Hering (1S29) was to inject into the jugular vein of one side a solution of potassium ferrocyanid, and then from time to time specimens of blood were taken from the jugular vein of the opposite side. The first specimen in which the ferrocyanid could be detected by its reaction with iron salts gave the least time necessary for a complete circuit. The method was subsequently improved in its technical details by Vierordt, and such results as the following were obtained : Dog, 16.32 seconds; horse, 28.8 seconds; rabbit, 7.46 seconds ; man (calculated), 23 seconds. The time required is less in the small than in the large animals, and Hering and Vierordt con- cluded that in general it requires from 26 to 28 beats of the heart to effect a complete circulation. Stewart has devised a simpler and better method,* based upon the electrical conductivity of the blood. If a solution of a neutral salt, such as sodium chlorid, more concen- t ruled than the blood, is injected into the circulation, the con- ductivity of the blood is increased. If the injection is made at a given moment and a portion of the vessel to be examined is properly connected with a galvanometer so as to measure the electrical conductivity through it, then the instant that the solution of salt reaches this latter vessel the fact will be indicated by a deflection of the galvanometer. I rsing this met hod, Stewart was able to show that in I he lesser circulat ion (I he pulmonary circuit) the velocity ♦ "Journal el' Physiology," 15, L, L894. VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 473 is very great compared with that of the systemic circulation — only about one-fifth of the time required for a complete circuit is spent in the lesser circulation. Attention may also be called to the fact that the important part of the circulation, as regards the nutritive activity of the blood, is the capillary path. It is while flowing through the capillaries that the chief exchange of gases and food material takes place. The average length of a capillary is estimated at 0.5 mm.; so that with a velocity of 0.5 mm. per second the average duration of the flow of any particle of blood through the capillary area is only about 1 sec. The Pressure Relations in the Vascular System. — That the blood is under different pressures in the several parts of the vascu- lar system has long been known and is easily demonstrated. When an artery is cut the blood flows out in a forcible stream and with spurts corresponding to the heart beats. When a large vein is wounded, on the contrary, although the blood flows out rapidly, the stream has little force. Exact measurements of the hydrostatic pressure under which the blood exists in the large arteries and veins were first published by Rev. Dr. Stephen Hales, an English clergy- man, in his famous book entitled " Statical Essays, containing Hsemostaticks," 1733.* This observer measured the static pressure of the blood in the arteries and veins by the simplest direct method possible. After tying the femoral artery in a horse he connected it to a glass tube 9 feet in length. On opening the vessel the blood mounted in the tube to a height of 8 feet 3 inches, showing that normally in the closed artery the blood is under a tension or pressure sufficient to support the weight of a column of blood of this height. A similar experiment made upon the vein showed a rise of only 12 inches. Methods of Recording Blood-pressure. — Since Hales's work the chief improvements in method which have marked and caused the development of this part of the subject have been the application of the mercury manometer by Poiseuillef (1828), the invention of the recording manometer and kymographion by LudwigJ (1847), and the later numerous improvements by many physiologists, and latterly the development of methods for measuring blood-pressures directly in man. The Hales method of measuring arterial pressure directly in terms of a column of blood is inconvenient on account of the great height, large fluctuations, and rapid clotting. The two former disadvantages are overcome by using a column of mer- cury. Since this metal is 13.5 times as heavy as blood, the column which will be supported by the blood will be correspondingly shorter * For an account of the life and works of this physiologist see Dawson, "The Johns Hopkins Hospital Bulletin," vol. xv, Nos. 159 to 161, 1904. f Poiseuille, "Recherches sur la force du coeur aortique." Paris, 1828. t Ludwig, "Mailer's Archiv f. Anatomie, Physiologie, etc.," 1847, p. 242. 474 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. and all the fluctuations will be similarly reduced. Poiseuille placed the mercury in a U tube of the general form shown in Fig. Is7. M. One leg was connected with the interior of an artery by appropriate tubing filled with liquid and when the clamp was removed from the vessel its pressure displaced the mercury in the limbs by a certain amount. The difference in height between the levels of the mercury in the two limbs in each experiment gives the blood pressure, which is therefore usually expressed as being equal to so many millimeters of mercury. By this expression it is meant that the pressure within the arter}^ is able to support a column Fig. 187. — A, Schema to show the recording mercury manometer and its connection with the artery: M, The manometer with the position of the mercury represented in black (the pressure is Riven by the distance in millimeters between the levels 1 and 2; one-half of this distance i- recorded on the kymographion by the pen, I'); F, the float resting upon the surface of the mercury ; (J, the cap through which the stem carrying the pen moves; E, offset for driving air out of the manometer and for filling or washing out the tube to the artery; K, the receptacle containing the soJ ition of sodium carbonate; c, the cannula for insertion into the artery; »■. the wa ooul arrangement shown in detail in />'. /;, The washout cannula: c, the glass cannula inserted into the artery; r, the stem connected with the reservoir of carbonate olution; «. tin; stem connected with the manom- eter. The arrows hoy, the currenl of carbonate solution during the process of washing out, the artery at that time being closed by a clamp. of mercury that many millimeters in height, and by multiplying this value by L3.5 the pressure can be obtained, when desirable, in terms of a column of blood or wider. For continuous obser- vations and permanent records the height of the column of mercury and its variations during an experiment are recorded by the device represented in Fig. 1H7 VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 475 The distal limb of the U tube in which the mercury rises carries a float of hard rubber, aluminum, or some other substance lighter than the mercury. The float in turn bears an upright steel wire which at the end of the glass tube plays through a small opening in a metal or glass cap. At its free end it bears a pen to trace the record. If smoked paper is used the pen is simply a smooth- pointed glass or metal arm, while if white paper is employed the wire carries a small glass pen with a capillary tube, which writes the record in ink. The tube connecting the proximal end of the manometer to the artery of the ani- mal must be filled with a solution that retards the coagulation of blood. For this purpose one employs ordinarily a saturated solution of sodium carbonate and bicarbonate. This tube is connected also by a T piece to a reservoir con- taining the carbonate solution, and by varying the height of this latter the pressure in the tube and the manometer may be adjusted beforehand to the pressure that is supposed or known to exist in the artery under experiment. By this means the blood, when connections are made with the manometer, does not penetrate far into the tube, and clotting is thereby delayed. In long observations it is most convenient to use what is known as a washout cannula, — the structure of which is represented in Fig. 187, B. When this instrument is attached to the cannula inserted into the blood-vessel one can, after first clamping off the artery, wash out the connections between the artery and the manometer with fresh carbonate solution as often as desired. By such means continuous records of arterial pressure may be obtained during many hours. Determinations of the pressure in the veins may be made with a similar apparatus, but owing to the low values that prevail on this side of the circulation it is more convenient to use some form of water manometer and thus record the venous pressures in terms of the height of the water column supported. It should be added also that when it is necessary to know the pressure in any special artery or vein the connections of the manometer are made usually to a side branch opening more or less at right angles into the vessel under investigation, or if this is not possible then a T" tube is inserted and the manometer is connected with the side branch. The reason for this procedure is that if the artery itself is ligated and the manometer is con- nected with its central stump, the flow in it and its dependent system of capil- laries and veins is cut off; the stump of the artery constitutes simply a con- tinuation of the tube from the manometer and serves as a side connection to the intact artery from which it arises. Thus, when a manometer is inserted into the carotid artery the pressure that is measured is the side-pressure in the innominate or aorta from which it arises, while a cannula in the central stump of a femoral artery measures the pressure in the iliac. A specimen of what is known as a blood-pressure record is shown in Fig. 188. The exact pressure at any instant, in millimeters of mercury, is obtained by measuring the distance between the base line and the record and multiplying by 2. The base line represents the position of the recording pen when it is at its zero position for the conditions of the experiment. It is necessary to mul- tiply the distance between the base line and the record by 2 because, as is seen in Fig. 187, the recording apparatus measures only the rise of the mercury in one limb of the manometer; there is, of course, an equal fall in the other limb. The blood-pressure record (Fig. 188) shows usually large rhyth- mical variations corresponding to the respiratory movements and in addition smaller waves caused by the heart beat. The causes of the respiratory waves of pressure are discussed in the section on respi- ration. Regarding the heart waves or pulse waves the usual record obtained by means of a mercury manometer gives an entirely false picture of the extent of the variations in pressure caused by the heart beat. The mass of mercury possesses considerable weight and iner- tia, which unfits it for following accurately very rapid changes in pressure. When the pressure changes are slow, as in the case of 476 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. the long respiratory waves seen in the record, the manometer un- doubtedly indicates their extent with entire accuracy. But when these changes are very rapid, as in the beat of a dog's or rabbit's heart, the mercury does not register either extreme in the variation, but tends to record the mean or average pressure. The full extent of the variations in arterial pressure caused by the heart beat can be AA^vV'/VVYV A.VvVV^'VV,V,A/V'V^y\''1V ] -'in. 188. — Typical blood pressure record with mercury manometer: Bp, The record showing (Ik- hcari heal and the larger curves due to (he respirations (respiratory waves nf blooa pre ure) and -t ill longer waves due to vasomotor changes; T, the time line, giving the time in seconds. The actual arterial pressure at any moment is the distance from the I,-, e line I bal i . I be line of zero pressure to I be blood-pressure hoe, multiplied by two. The ■• value are indicated in the vertical line drawn to the right, winch shows that the pre ure al the time of the experiment was 100 nous. Hg_. The small size of the ion in pre ure due to each heart heat, b altogether a false picture due to the inertia of the mercury, it inability to follow completely the quick change. Kach heart heat, in lead of being mailer, bould be larger than the respiratory waves. determined by other means (see below), and, if the knowledge thus obtained is applied to the con-eel ion of the record of the mercury manometer, the tracing given in Fig. 188 should have, so far as the heart beats are concerned, somewhat the appearance shown in big. is!). This hitter figure gives a more accurate mental picture of the actual conditions of pressure in the large arteries, as influenced by VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 477 the heart beat. These arteries are, in fact, subject to very rapid and very extensive changes in pressure at each beat of the heart, and these changes are naturally more pronounced when the force of the heart beat is increased, — for instance, by muscular exercise. Systolic, Diastolic, and Mean Arterial Pressure. — As stated in the last paragraph, the arterial pressure in the larger arteries undergoes extensive variations with each heart beat. The maxi- mum pressure caused by the systole of the heart, the apex of the pulse wave, is spoken of as systolic pressure; the minimum pressure in the artery — that is, the pressure at the end of the diastole of the heart, or the bottom of the pulse- wave, is known as the diastolic pressure. In a dog under ordinary conditions of experimentation the systolic (lateral) pressure in the aorta may be as much as 168 mms., while the diastolic pressure is only 100 mms. In man the Fig. 189. — Schematic representation of the pressure change caused by each heart beat. The schema represents three heart beats supposed to be recorded on a rapidly moving surface by a manometer delicate enough to follow the pressure changes accurately. The top of the pulse wave measures the systolic pressure; the bottom the diastolic pressure. systolic pressure as measured in the brachial artery may be taken in round numbers as equal to 110 to 116 mms., while the diastolic pressure is only 65 to 75 mms. The difference between the systolic and the diastolic pressure has been designated con- veniently as the pulse pressure. It measures, of course, the variation in pressure in any given artery caused by the heart beat, and so far as that artery is concerned it gives the force of the heart beat except for the small component used to accelerate the movement of the blood. From the figures given above it will be seen that the pulse pressure in the brachial artery of man averages 45 mms. Hg. Each systole of the heart distends this artery, therefore, by a sudden increase in pressure equal to the weight of a column of mercury 45 mms. high. As we go outward in the arterial tree the pulse pressure becomes less and less, the oscillations in pressure with each heart beat are less marked, until, finally, in the smallest arteries and 47S CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. capillaries and in the veins there is no pulse wave, and no difference between systolic and diastolic pressure. In speaking of the pressure in the blood-vessels we refer usually to what is called the mean pressure. It is obvious that, so far as the larger arteries are con- cerned, the mean pressure is only a convenient expression for the average pressure during a certain period. If, by the methods described below, we determine the systolic and diastolic pressures in the artery of a man, and assume that there has been no general variation between the two observations, we can estimate the mean pressure with approximate accuracy by taking the arithmetical mean of the two figures. The arithmetical mean of systolic and diastolic pressures during any given heart-beat does not give the true mean pressure, owing to the form of the pulse wave (see Fig. 211). If the rise from diastolic to systolic pressure and the succeeding fall took place uniformly, so that the pulse curve constituted J it;. 190. — Schema to indicate the general relations of systolic, mean, and diastolic- pressures throughout the arterial system : s. Systolic; m, mean; (/.diastolic; c, pressure at beginning of the capillaries. The distance from a tod represents the pulse pressure at different parts of the arterial system. a true triangle, the true mean pressure would be given by the arithmetical mean of the two pressures. As a matter of fact, the descending limb of the pulse curve is not a straight but a curved line, and it is broken, moreover, by secondary waves. The position of the mean pressure during any given heart-beat will vary, therefore, with the form of Ihc pulse curve. Generally speaking, it lies nearer to the diastolic than to the systolic level.* In physiological observations, as a rule, no attempt is made to estimate Ihe mean pressure for any given time with mathematical accuracy. In the ordinary tracing as given by the mercury man- ometer (Fig. L88) the mean pressure for any given period during which the variations have been symmetrical and not extreme is estimated as the arithmetical mean of the highest and lowest point- ic'ielifd. When desirable, the mean pressure may be recorded by introducing a resistance (narrowing the tube by means of :i stopcock) between the artery and the manometer. The latter * Sec Dawson, " British Medical Journal," 1906, 996. VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 479 will then record mean pressure and show no variations with the heart beat. A general idea of the variations in systolic, diastolic, and mean pressures, throughout the arterial system, may be ob- tained from the schema given in Fig. 190. Method of Measuring Systolic and Diastolic Pressure in Animals. — In animals a manometer may be connected directly with the artery and systolic and diastolic pressures may be obtained in one of two general ways: (1) By using some form of pressure recorder or manometer sufficiently mobile to follow very quick changes of pressure. (2) By using a mercury manometer pro- vided with maximum and minimum valves. Of the manometers that have been divised to register accurately the quick changes in pressure due to the heart beat, the two that are most frequently Fig. 191. — The spring manometer of Fick (after Langendorff) : f, The flat metal tube filled with liquid; r, the lead tube connecting with the artery; h, h, h, the lever mechanism of light wood communicating the movements of / to the writing point, s; p, a small disc immersed in a vessel of oil to still further dampen the inertia swings. referred to in physiological literature are the spring manometer of Fick* and the membrane manometer of Hurthle.f The Fick manometer is shown in Fig. 191. It consists of a flat, hollow metallic spring bent into the form of a C. The interior is filled with liquid and is connected by rigid tubing, also filled with liquid, with the interior of the artery. The variations of pressure in the artery are transmitted to the interior of the spring and tend to straighten it, thus causing corresponding movements of the free end. Before or after using this instrument it must be calibrated, — that is, the variations in movement must be given absolute values in terms of millimeters of mercury by ascertaining directly the extent of move- ment caused by known pressures. The Hiirthle manometer is more frequently used at present. The principle made use of in this instrument is illustrated by the diagram in Fig. 192. The instrument consists essentially of a small box or tambour of very limited capacity; the top of the tambour is covered * Fick, "Archiv f. Physiologie," 1864, p. 583. t "Archiv f. d. gesammte Physiologie," 49, 45, 1891. 4S0 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. Fig. 192. — Diagram showing construction of Htirthle's manometer. — (After Cvrtis.) The interior of the heart or the artery is connected by rigid tubing to a very small tambour, 2'. The tubing and the tambour are filled with liquid. The movements of the rubber dam covering the tambour are greatly magnified by a compound lever, S. _ The tendency of this lever to "fling" may be prevented by an arrangement not shown in the diagram. The essential principles of the recorder are, first, liquid conduction from heart to tambour; second, a very small tambour and membrane so that a minimal volume of liquid escapes from the heart into the tambour. To the arteri/- ^^ Fig. 1&3. — Schema to illustrate the use of valves in determining maximum (systolic) and minimum (diastolic) blood-pros ill*-. When :;topeock n i:i open the heart beats are trail milted through the maximum valve ami flic mercury in llie manometer i prevented from falling bel ween beats. The manometer will record I be highest pressure reached during the period "i ob i rvation. The reverse oooure when valve 6 alone is open. VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 481 with thin rubber dam and the cavity is filled with liquid and connected by- rigid tubing, also filled with liquid, with the interior of the artery or heart. Variations in pressure in the artery are transmitted through the column of liquid to the rubber membrane of the tambour, and the movements of this latter are greatly magnified by a sensitive lever attached to it. The liquid conduction and the small size of the tambour, which prevents any notice- able outflow of liquid, combine to give a sensitive and very prompt record of pressure changes. It is also necessary to calibrate this instrument whenever used in order to give absolute values to the records obtained. A specimen of a blood-pressure record obtained with this instrument is shown in Fig. 194. It will be noticed that the size of the heart beat, relative to the distance from the base line is much greater than in the record obtained with the mercury manometer, Fig. 188. Fig. 194. — Blood-pressure record from a dog with a Hiirthle manometer. The size of the heart beats is relatively much greater than with a mercury manometer. In this case the systolic pressure is about 150 mms. Hg; the diastolic, 100 mms. ; and the heart beat or pulse pressure, 50 mms. The method that depends upon the use of maximum and minimum valves may be understood by reference to Fig. 193. On the path between the artery and the manometer one may place a maximum and a minimum valve so ar- ranged that the blood-pressure and heart beat may be transmitted through either valve. As is shown by the figure, if the connection is maintained through the maximum valve for a certain time the highest pressure reached during that period will be recorded, while, when the minimum valve is used the lowest pressure reached will be indicated. Such valves, of course, act slowly and can not be used to determine the maximum and minimum pressure in the artery during a single heart beat; they record the highest and lowest point reached during a certain given interval. Actual Data as to the Mean Pressure in Arteries, Veins, and Capillaries. — The mean value of the pressure in the aorta has been determined for many mammals. It is found that the actual figures vary with the conditions under which the results have been obtained. Such values as the following may be quoted:* Horse 321 mms. to 150 mms. Hg. Dog 172 " "104 " " Sheep 206 " "156 " " Cat 150 " Rabbit 108 " " 90 " " Man (probable, Tigerstedt) 150 " *See Volkmann, "Die Haemodynamik," 1850. 31 482 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. It appears from these figures that there is no proportion between the size of an animal and the amount of mean arterial pressure. It is probable that there may be a general relationship between the size of the animal — that is. the size of the heart — and the amount Jb C- M R F p 5 IBO \ \ \ IM \ \ \ S IfC ■> \u tt it r IM . i Vf 'O -n / 100 -/ a if s no M 4(> 10 0 Fig. 195 — Curve showing the results of actual measurement of systolic, diastolic, and mean pressure (lateral pressures) along the aorta and femoral of the dog. The branches through which the lateral pressures were obtained are indicated as follows: Sb, Left sub- clavian; C-M, celiac arid superior mesenteric; R, left renal; F, left femoral (KHenberger and Bauni), external iliac; P, profunda branch of femoral; 8, saphena. The pressure in millimeters is given along the ordinate's to the left. It will lie noted that the mean and the dia tolic pre ures remain practically the same throughout the descending aorta and into the femoral. The systolic pressure shows a marked increase at the lower end of the aorta and then falls off rapidly. The pulse pressure at the inferior end of the descend- ing aorta ia much larger than at the arch. (Dawaon.) of pulse pressure or the oscillation of pressure with each heart beat, but sufficient data are not, at hand to determine this point. As we pass from the aorta to the smaller arteries the mean pressure decreases somewhat, although not very rapidly, while the pulse pressure decreases also and to a more noticeable extent. VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 483 This fact is illustrated in Fig. 195, which gives a graphic representation of a number of experimental determinations* of systolic and diastolic pressures in the large arteries of the dog. If we turn to the other end of the vascular system, the veins, we find that the lowest pressure is in the venae cavse and that it increases gradually as we go toward the capillary area. Accord- ing to one observer, f the fall in pressure from periphery toward the heart is at the rate of 1 mm. Hg for every 35 mms. of distance. We" have such figures as the following: Dog (Opitz). Sheep. Superior vena cava (near Jugular vein 0.2 mm. Hg. auricle) = — 2.96 mms. Hg. Facial vein . 3.0 mms. " Superior vena cava more Branch of brachial ... 9.0 " " distal = —1.38 " " Crural 11.4 " " External jugular (left) . . = 0.52 mm. Right brachial = 3.90 mms Left facial = 5.12 " Left femoral .......= 5.39 " Left saphenous = 7.42 " Fig. 196. — Schematic representation of the general relations of blood-pressure (side pressure) in different parts of the vascular system: a. The arteries; c, the capillaries; v, the veins. The mean and diastolic pressures remain nearly constant in the arterial system, as far as they can be measured accurately. The pressures in the veins are represented as uniform at any one point. In the large veins near the heart there are variations of pressure with each respiration and with each heart beat (Venous Pulse, p. 513). At the heart, therefore, the pressure of the blood upon the walls of the veins is nearly nil, and, indeed, owing to the circumstance that the large veins lie in the thoracic cavity, in which the pres- sure is below that of the atmosphere, the tension of the blood in them may also be below atmospheric pressure, although doubt- less at this point (vena cava) the pressure within the vein is greater, certainly not less than the pressure on its exterior (intrathoracic pressure). To complete the general conception of the pressure relations in the vascular system it is necessary to know the pressure of the blood in the smallest arteries and veins and in the capillaries. It is not possible — in the cases of the capillaries, for instance — to connect a manometer directly * Dawson, "American Journal of Physiology," 15, 244, 1906. t Burton-Opitz, "American Journal of Physiology," 9, 198, 1903. 4S4 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. with the vessels, and recourse has been had to a less direct and certain method. The pressure in the capillaries in dif- ferent regions of the skin has been estimated by determining the pressure necessary to obliterate them — that is, to blanch the skin. A glass plate is laid upon the skin or mucous membrane and weights are added until a distinct change in the color of the skin is noted.* Knowing the necessary weight to produce this effect and the area submitted to compression, the pressure may be expressed in terms of millimeters of mercury or blood. The following example may be used to illustrate this method: Suppose that the glass plate has an area of 4 sq.mms., and that to blanch the skin under it a weight of 1 gm. is necessary; 1 gm. of water = 1 c.c. or 1000 c mms. Therefore to blanch this area would require a column of water contain- ing 1000 c.mms. with a cross-area of 4 sq.mms. The height of this column would therefore be equal to —V" or 250 mms. of water, — that is, 18.5 mms. Hg. The results obtained by this method are not very constant and can only be considered as approximate. It would appear, how- ever, that the pressure lies somewhere between 20 and 40 mms. of mercury. Thus, upon the gums of a rabbit von Kries found a capillary pressure of 33 mms. Hg. By means of a more adjustable instrument von Reckling- hausenf estimates that in man the pressure within the capil- laries of the finger-tips or, to be more accurate, within the small arteries supplying these capillaries, is equal to 55 mms. Hg. (See p. 491.) The general relations of the pressures in arteries, veins, and capillaries may be expressed in a curve such as is shown in Fig. 196. It should be added that in this curve and in all the figures so far quoted in regard to the actual pressure within the different arteries and veins, it is assumed that the animal is in a recumbent posture. In an animal standing upon his feet, especially in .ui upright animal like man, it is obvious that the effect of gravity will modify greatly the actual figures of pressure. Upon the arteries and veins of the feet, for example, there will be exerted a hydrostatic pressure equal to the height of the column of liquid between the feet and the heart, which adds itself to the pressure resulting from the circulation as caused by the heart. When the animal is in a recumbent position the hydro- static factor practically disappears. (See p. 500.) * V. Kries, "Beriehte d. Sachs. Geaellflchaft d. Wiss. Math.-phys. Classe," 1875, p. I is. f Von Recklinghausen, "Archiv f. exp. Path. u. Pharnak.," 55, :'>7.r>, l(.)07. VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 485 The Method of Determining Blood-pressure in the Large Arteries of Man. — It is a matter of interest and practical impor- tance to ascertain even approximately the arterial pressure in man and its variations in health and disease. The first practical method for determining this point upon man was suggested by von Basch (1887), who devised an instrument for this purpose, the sphygmo- manometer. Since that time a number of different instruments have been described, but attention may be called to two only, which are among the most recent and convenient. In the first place, it must be clearly recognized that the arterial pressure in the large arteries of man shows marked variations with the heart beat; the pressure during the beat of the heart rises suddenly to a much higher level than during the diastole. The relation of the systolic (or maximum) and diastolic (or minimum) pressures is indicated by the Fig. 197. — Figure of the Riva-Rocci apparatus (Sahli): a, The leather collar with inside rubber bag to go on the arm ; c, the bulb for blowing up the rubber bag and thus compressing the artery; d, the manometer dipping into the reservoir of mercury, 6, to meas- sure the amount of pressure. diagram in Fig. 190. The instruments that have been invented for determining human blood-pressure are in reality adapted, more or less accurately, to determine one or the other or both of these pres- sures. No instrument has been devised for determining the mean pressure, and, indeed, from a physiological standpoint such an instrument would not be so valuable as one that gives us the figures corresponding to the systolic and the diastolic pressures and thus allows us to calculate an approximate mean. For it is evident that in the latter case we should be in possession of more data with which to analyze the causes for any given variation in pressure. The principle of determining the systolic pressure alone •486 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. is very simple: it consists in determining the amount of pressure necessary to completely obliterate the artery, — that is, to prevent a pulse from passing through the region under compression. This principle was used originally by von Basch, but its application has been made perhaps most successfully in the simple apparatus suggested by Riva-Rocci, which is adapted especially for measure- ments of pressure in the brachial artery. One form of this instru- ment is represented in Fig. 197. The leather or canvas band, a, is buckled snugly around the arm. On the inner surface of this band there is a rubber bag which communicates with the mercury manometer, d, and the pressure bulb, c. When the band is in place rhythmical compressions of c will force air into the rubber bag surround- ing the arm. This bag is blown up and exerts pressure upon the arm and through the arm tissue upon the brachial artery. The amount of pressure that is being exerted upon the arm is indicated at any moment by the mer- c Fig. 198. — Schema to illustrate the fact that when the pressure upon the outside of the artery is equal to the diastolic pressure the pulse wave will cause a maximal expansion of the artery: n represents the normal artery distended by diastolic blood-pressure; the dotted lines indicate the additional expansion caused by the pulse wave; b represents the artery when compressed by an outside pressure equal to the diastolic pressure within; the artery then takes the size of an empty artery kept patent by the rigidity of its walls. The pulse wave, on reaching this section, finds a relaxed wall and causes, therefore, a maximum extension. cury manometer. The moment of obliteration of the artery is determined by feeling for recording) the pulse in the radial artery. The moment that this pulse disappears, as the pressure upon the brachial is raised, indicates the maximum or systolic pressure in the brachial artery. As the pressure is low- ered again the pulse reappears. Among other sources of error involved in this method it is to be remembered that the tactile sensibility is not sufficiently delicate to detect a minimal pulse in the artery. Other methods of determin- ing the systolic pressure (see below) indicate, as a matter of fact, that the pulse continues some time after an individual of average tactile sensibility is unable to detect it. To determine the diastolic pressure is more difficult and requires some- what more apparatus. The principle employed was first suggested by Marey and first practically applied by Mosso.* The method consists in -ecoiding by some means the pulsations of the artery under different pressures aiid de- termining under what pressure the maximal pulsations are given. Thie pres- sure should be equal to the diastolic pressure within the artery. The prin- ciple involved may be illustrated by the accompanying figure (Fig. 198). Let a represent a longitudinal section of an artery distended by normal diastolic arterial pressure. At each heart beat the force of the pulse will dis- tend the artery still more, as represented by the dotted lines, and this in- crease in size may he measured by proper transmitting apparatus. If now pressure is brought to bear upon the outside of the artery its lumen will be diminished as the outside pressure is increased, and when this pres- sure is equal to the diastolic blood-pressure within the artery one will neu- i ralize I he ol her, and the diameter of the artery will be equal to that assumed when the vessel contains Mood under no pressure and is kept, patent only by the stiffness of its walls (b). Under this condition the pulse wave? when it traverses this portion of the vessel finds its walls completely relaxed, as it and the force of the heart wave will consequently cause a greater dis- * "Archives italiennes de biologic," 23, 177, L895. VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 487 tension of the arterial walls and a larger pulse wave in the recording ap- paratus. If the outside pressure is increased beyond the amount of diastolic pressure it will not only neutralize this latter, but will tend to overcome the stiffness of the arterial wall. When the pulse wave passes through this stretch it will be forced not only to distend the walls, but also to overcome the excess Fig. 199. — Record {Erlanger) to show the maximum size of the recorded pulse wave when the outside or extra vascular pressure is equal to the internal diastolic pressure. The artery is compressed first with a pressure above systolic, sufficient to obliterate the lumen. As this pressure is lowered in steps of 5 mms. the recorded pulse wave increases in size to a maximum and then again becomes smaller. The outside pressure with which the maximum pulse is obtained measures the amount of the internal diastolic pressure (Marey's principle). of pressure on the outside. The movement of the walls with the pulse wave will be less extensive in proportion to the excess of pressure on the outside. If, therefore, one starts with an outside pressure sufficient to obliterate the artery completely the recorded pulse wave will be small. As this pressure Fig. 200. — Schema showing the construction of the Erlanger apparatus: a. Rubber bag of the arm piece ; c, bulb for blowing up this bag and putting pressure on the arm ; 6, the manometer for measuring the pressure ; i, two-way stopcock (when turned so as to communicate with the capillary opening, A;, it allows the pressure in a to fall slowly) ; e, a rubber bag in a glass chamber, /; e communicates with a when stopcock d is open and the pulse waves from a are transmitted to e; the pulsations of e in turn are transmitted to the delicate tambour, h, and are thus recorded. is diminished, the pulse waves become larger up to a certain point and then decrease again in size (see Fig. 199). The outside pressure at which this maximum pulse is obtained measures, according to the principle stated above, the diastolic pressure within the artery. That the principle is correct has 4ss CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. been shown by direct experiments upon the exposed artery of a dog, in which the pressure was measured by the method outlined above and also directly by a manometer connected with the interior of the artery.* In such experi- ments upon man, however, one condition is present which detracts from the absolute value of the results obtained, although, since it is substantially a constant factor, it does not seriously interfere with relative results, that is, with observations upon the variations of pressure under different condi- tions. This source of error lies in the fact that in the living person the out- side pressure can not be applied directly to the arteries, but only indirectly through the intervening tissues. These tissues interpose a certain resistance to the pressure exerted from without, and some of this pressure must be spent Fig. 201.- Erlanger apparatus, The collar for the arm is not shown. The parts maybe understood by reference to the Bchema given in Fig. 200. in overcoming this resistance. The amount of the resistance offered by the tissues lias been estimated differently by various authors, but probably lies between •» and 10 nuns, of mercury, — that is, the pressure as measured exceeds the real diastolic pressure by this amount. Several instruments have been devised, according to this principle, to measure diastolic pressures, but the sphygmomanometer described by Erlangerf is probably the most complete * Howell and Brush, " Proceedings of the Massachusetts Medical Society," 1901. ■(■"American Journal of Physiology," "Proceedings of the American Physiological Society," 6, xxii., 1902; and " Johns Hopkins Hospital Reports," 12,53, 1 «.J04. VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 489 and the most convenient for actual use. This instrument is illustrated in Figs. 200 and 201. It may be used to determine both systolic and diastolic pressure. The way in which the apparatus is used may be understood from the sche- matic Fig. 200. a is the rubber bag which is buckled upon the arm by a leather strap. This bag communicates with the mercury manometer, b, with a pres- sure bag, c, through the two-way stopcock, i, and through the stopcock d with a rubber bag, e, contained in a glass chamber, /. This glass chamber com- municates above with a sensitive tambour, h, and by means of the stopcock g can be placed in communication with the outside air. The systolic pressure may be determined in two ways: By one method only the mercury manom- eter is necessary, the instrument corresponding with the Riva-Rocci appa- ratus described above. By means of the pressure bag, c, the bag, a, upon the arm is blown up until the pressure is above the systolic pressure and the radial pulse below disappears. By turning stopcock i properly the system is allowed to communicate with the air through a capillary opening, k. Consequently the pressure upon the artery in the arm falls slowly, and by palpating the radial artery one can determine the pressure, as measured by the mercury lalP^ Fig. 202. — To show the method of detecting the systolic pressure upon the tracing given by the Erlanger sphygmomanometer. The pressure upon the arm is dropping slowly and at the point just before the mark x the pulse wave shows the spreading of the limbs that indicates the first pulse wave to get through the occluded artery. The next pulse wave shows the sudden increase in size of the pulse, which has also been taken to mark the systolic pressure. manometer, at which the pulse just gets through. This pressure will measure approximately the systolic pressure. The second method (method of v. Recklinghausen) gives higher and doubtless more accurate results. In this method the pressure is at first raised above systolic pressure with stopcocks d and g open, a, e, and b are under the same pressure. If stopcock g is now turned off, the pulsations in a are transmitted to e and through it to the tambour, h, and the lever of the tambour writes these pulsations on a kymo- graphion. It should be explained that pulsations are obtained even when the pressure on the arm is much more than sufficient to completely obliterate the brachial artery. The reason for this is that the pulsations of the central stump of the closed artery will be communicated to bag a. When the pressure is suprasystolic these pulsations are small. If now the pressure in the system is diminished slowly by turning stopcock i so as to communicate with the capillary opening, k, it will be found that at a certain point the pulsations suddenly increase in height (Fig. 202). This point marks the moment when the pulse wave is first able to break through the brachial artery, and it gives, therefore, the systolic pressure. In many cases this method of determining the point of systolic pressure is not satisfactory, since the pulse waves increase 490 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. gradually in amplitude without a sudden break, or perhaps there is more than one place at which a sudden increase occurs. A more reliable method according to Erlanger is to note the point at which the ascending and descend- ing limbs of the pulse wave show a noticeable separation (Fig. 202). "At the moment the pressure on the artery falls below systolic, blood succeeds in making its way beneath the cuff. This must be squeezed out before the lever can return to the base line, whereas at higher pressures the lever is raised only through the hydraulic-ram action of the pulse wave upon the upper edge of the cuff." After finding the systolic pressure the diastolic pressure is obtained by allowing the pressure to drop still further. The pulsations increase in height to a maximum size and then decrease. The pressure at which the maximum pulse wave is obtained marks the diastolic pressure. It is better perhaps in dropping the pressure for this last purpose to manipu- late stopcock i so as to drop the pressure 5 mms. at a time, recording the pulse wave at each pressure. In this way a record is obtained such as is given in Fig. 199. It should be added, also, that in order to keep the lever of the tambour horizontal while the pressure in the system is being lowered there is a minute pinhole in the metal bottom of the tambour. Through this pinhole the pressure in the tambour and chamber, /, is kept atmospheric throughout, except during the quick changes caused by the pulse waves. By means of this instrument one can determine within a minute or so the amount of the systolic and diastolic pressure in the brachial artery, and also, of course, the difference between the two, the pulse pressure, which may be taken as a measure of the force of the heart beat. The Normal Arterial Pressure in Man and its Variations.— By means of one or other of the instruments devised for the purpose, numerous results have been obtained regarding the blood-pressure in man at different ages and under varying normal and abnormal conditions. Unfortunately the methods used have not always been complete. Some authors give only systolic pressures, for example. In such experiments also a troublesome factor is always the psychical element. The mental interest that the individual experimented upon takes in the procedure almost always causes a rise of pressure and perhaps a changed heart rate. Results, as a rule, upon any individual show lower values after the novelty of the procedure has worn off and the patient submits to the process as an uninteresting routine. It should be remembered also that in measuring arterial pressures in man the measurements must always be made at the level of the heart, as is usually done, the brachial artery being selected, or if other arteries are employed, an allow- ance must be made for- differences in level. (See paragraph on the Eydrostatic Effect, p. 500.) Under normal conditions Potain* estimated the systolic pressure in the radial of the adult at about 170 nuns, of mercury and the variations for different ages he expressed in the following figures: Age (i III I.") 20 25 30 40 50 60 SO Pressure (systolic). 89 135 L50 170 ISO 190 200 210 220 * " La prewiou arterielle de L'homme," Paris, 1902. VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 491 Without the other side of the picture — that is, the diastolic pres- sure and the force of the heart beat (pulse pressure) — it is difficult to interpret these figures. The rapid increase up to maturity probably represents chiefly the larger output of blood from the heart ; the slower and more regular increase from maturity to old age is due possibly to the gradual hardening of the arteries, since the less elastic the arteries become, the greater will be the systolic rise with each heart beat. With his more complete apparatus Erlanger reports that in the adult (20 to 25), when the psychical factor is excluded, the average pressure in the brachial is 110 mms., systolic, and 65 mms., diastolic, — figures much lower than those given by Potain. Von Recklinghausen's figures for the same artery are, systolic pressure 116 mms. Hg, diastolic pressure 73 mms. Hg. Erlanger and Hooker report observations upon the effect of meals, of baths, of posture, the diurnal rhythm, etc.* The effect of meals is particularly instructive in that it illustrates admirably the play of the compensatory mechanisms of the circu- lation by means of which the heart and the blood-vessels are ad- justed to each other's activity. During a meal there is a dilatation of the blood-vessels in the abdominal area, or, as it is frequently called in physiology, the splanchnic area, since it receives its vasomotor fibers through the splanchnic nerve. The natural effect of this dilatation, if the other factors of the circulation remained constant, would be a fall of pressure in the aorta and a diminution in blood-flow to other organs, such as the skin and the brain. This tendency seems to be compensated, however, by an increased output of blood from the heart. Observations with the sphygmomanometer show that after full meals there is a marked increase in the pulse pressure, indicating a more forcible beat of the heart. So far as the effect on the heart is concerned, the result of a meal is similar to that of muscular exercise, and this reaction may account for the fact, not infrequently observed, that in elderly people whose arteries are rigid an apoplectic stroke may follow a heavy meal. The Method of Determining Venous Pressures and Capillary Pressures in Man. — A number of methods have been proposed for determining venous pressures in man, the simplest being that described by Gaertner.f It consists simply in raising slowly the arm of the patient until the veins on the back of the hand just disappear. The height above the heart at which this occurs gives the venous pressure in the right auricle, since the vein may be considered as a manometer tube ending in the auricle. In this and in other methods of measuring venous ♦Erlanger and Hooker, "The Johns Hopkins Hospital Reports," vol. xii., 1904. f'Muench. mediz. Wochenschrift," 1903, 1904. 492 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. pressures, and the same is true, of course, of arterial and capillary pressures, there must be some agreement as to what constitutes the heart-level, since the highest and lowest points of the heart when the individual is standing or sitting may differ by as much as 15 centimeters, von Recklinghausen proposes the level made by a dorso ventral line drawn from the bottom of the sternum (costal angle) to the spinal column. This authorf has devised a simple apparatus for determining venous and capillary pressures, the principle of which is shown by the schema repre- sented in Fig. 203. A circular bag of thin rubber with a diameter of about 5J cm. is provided with a central opening of 2 cm. The bag is connected with a pump so that it can be blown up, and the degree of pressure exerted is measured by an attached manometer. This bag, moistened with glycerine, is laid upon a vein, as represented in the diagram. It is covered by a glass plate held firmly Fig. 203. — To illustrate the method of measuring venous pressure: //, The back of the hand in which a single vein is represented; B, the circular rubber bag with central opening, and with a tube, T, which leads to the pump and the manometer; G, glass plate held over the rubber bag. The bag, B, is blown up by pressure through the tube T until the vein is collapsed. The pressure at which this occurs, or the pressure at which the vein reappears as the bag is allowed to empty, gives the pressure within the vein. — (von Recklinghausen.) in posil ion and the bag is then blown up until the vein disappears; the pressure at which this happens is shown by the manometer and marks (lie pressure within i ho vein. A convenient modification of this apparatus which lias been described by Eyster and Hookerf is shown in Fig. 20 1. The box, />', used for compressing the vein is connected by rubber tubing will: a rubber manometer, Cf, and a pressure-bull), Ii. The structure of the pressure box is shown in the smaller figure. It. consists of an aluminum frame or box, the top and one side of which are made of glass. One of the sides is perforated by a tube which oonneets with the maiiomel er, as shown in I he larger figure. The frame is cut away on two sides, so thai when il is tied upon the arm the vein will not be compressed. Over the bottom of this frame is laid a thin sheet of rubber dam, :!, with a hole cut ii) I he center, and I he aluminum frame with its rubber bottom is then set into a close fitting brass frame, 2, which serves to keep the rubber membrane in place. When placed in position Upon the arm the rubber dam lies upon the vein and presses upon il as the pressure i rai ed in the box. The vein is observed through the glass top and the hole in the rubber, and the pressure at which it is just obliterated is read from the manometer. W'iib instruments of ibis kind the degree of pressure neces- sity to obliterate a given vein in the arm, band, or foot can * Von Recklinghausen, "Archiv f. exper. Pathol, u. Pharmakol, " 55, 470, l!ll)li. • rand Hooker, "The Johns Hopkins Hospital Bulletin," 274, L908. VELOCITY AND PRESSURE OF BLOOD-FLOW. 493 be determined readily in terms of a column of water, but it is obvious that for any given vein this pressure will vary with the position of the vein. When the hand hangs pendent at the side the pressure within its veins will be greater than when the hand is raised to the heart-level. The pressure actually measured for any given position of the hand or foot must, Fig. 204. — Apparatus for determining venous blood-pressure in man: B, The box with glass top for putting pressure on the vein; the details are shown in the small figure (Fig. 2), in which 1 show- the alumimum box; 2, the brass collar which fits over 1 and holds in place the perforated sheet of rubber dam; 3, which forms the bottom of the box and is forced down on the vein. E, pressure bulb for increasing pressure in the box until the vein is obliterated. G, water manometer to measure the pressure. (Eyster and Hooker.) therefore, be corrected for the heart-level by determining the vertical distance between the vein and the heart (costal angle), and subtracting this distance, expressed in centimeters, from the pressure, also expressed in centimeters, which was found necessary to obliterate the vein. Measurements made by this method and corrected for the heart-level show that in the normal 494 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. person the pressure within the small veins of the hand or arm may vary between 3 and 10 centimeters of water. Unusual or pathological conditions which cause a congestion in the venous side of the heart will raise the venous pressure correspondingly to 20 centimeters or more.* When the venous pressure is measured in the small veins of the feet in a person wliile standing we should suppose that after a reduction to the heart level it would be about the same as that noted for the veins of the hands, since the vessels are of about the same order with reference to their distance from the capillary bed. In a series of observations of this kind, reported by von Recklinghausen, it was found, on the contrary, that after subtracting the distance between the foot and the heart, the pressure within the veins was negative by as much as 40 cm. The author explains this unexpected result by supposing that the flow through the foot got up only enough pressure in the veins to lift the blood to the level of the pelvis, and that the complete closure of the venous valves at this level protected the veins from the full pressure of the column of blood. If the pressure was not sufficient to lift the blood to the heart, then evidently no flow was taking place in the foot. Eventually no doubt the pressure in the veins would have risen sufficiently to lift the blood to the heart-level and thus reestablish the flow, but it seems probable that under the ordinary condition's of life this result is effected by the cooperation of the muscles of the legs. The contractions of these muscles, aided by the venous valves, squeeze the blood upward to the heart. The fact that in standing quietly the flow through the feet may be suspended or impeded, for a time at least, throws some light, as von Recklinghausen suggests, upon the fact that it is so difficult to stand for any length of time without moving. The apparatus described above may be used for determining capillary as well as venous pressures, according to the principle described on p. 484. For this purpose the pressure box is laid upon a given skin area and the pressure is raised until the skin beneath is blanched. The pressure is then lowered slowly until the skin again reddens, showing the reestablishment of the capillary flow. The pressure thus obtained is corrected as described for the level of the heart, f * For a description of some pathological cases, see Eyster and Hooker, loc. cit. t For some technical details, see von Recklinghausen,, loc. cit. CHAPTER XXVI. THE PHYSICAL FACTORS CONCERNED IN THE PRO- DUCTION OF BLOOD-PRESSURE AND BLOOD- VELOCITY. In the preceding pages some of the essential facts have been stated regarding the pressure and the velocity of the blood in the different parts of the vascular system. We may now consider the physical factors that are responsible for the production and mainte- nance of these peculiarities. The problem as it actually exists in the circulation, with its elastic vessels varying in size from the aorta, with an internal diameter of nearly 20 mms., to the capil- laries, with a diameter of 0.009 mm., is extremely complex, but the general static and dynamic principles inyolved are simple and easily understood. Side Pressure and Velocity Pressure. — When water flows through a tube under, let us say, a constant head of pressure it encounters a resistance due to the friction between the walls of the vessel and the particles of water. This resistance will be greater, the narrower the tube. A part of the head of pressure used to drive the liquid along the tube will be used in overcoming this resistance to its movement, and the volume of the outflow will be correspond- ingly diminished. If we use an apparatus such as is represented in Fig. 205, consisting of a reservoir, H, and a long outflow tube, 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, the outflow from the end and the pressure along the tube may be measured directly. We must suppose that the head of pressure — that is, the height of the water in H — is kept constant by some means. The resistance or tension due to the friction in the tube may be measured at any point by inserting a side-tube or gauge (piezometer) at that point. The liquid will rise in this tube to a level corresponding to the pressure or resistance offered to the movement of the liquid at that point — that is, the weight of the column of liquid will measure the pressure at that point upon a surface corresponding to the cross-area of the tube. The pressure or tension at any point may be spoken of as the side pressure or lateral pressure, and it expresses the amount of resistance offered to the flow of the liquid because of the friction exerted upon the water by the walls of the tube between that point and the exit. This side pressure increases in a straight line from the point of exit 495 496 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. to the reservoir, and this in general is the picture presented by the circulation. The reservoir, the head of pressure, is represented by the aorta, the exit for the outflow by the opening of the venae cavae into the right auricle, and the side pressure or internal tension of the blood due to friction against the walls of the vessels increases from the venae cavae back to the aorta. If from aorta to vena cava the vessels were of the same diameter the increase would be in a straight line, as in the case of the model. In this model it will be noticed that the straight line showing the side pressure does not strike the top of the column of liquid in the reservoir, but corre- sponds to a certain height, W. This expresses the fact that, of the total head of pressure in the reservoir, which we may designate as H, a certain portion only, but a large portion, h' ', is used in over- Fig. 205. — Schema to illustrate the side pressure due to resistance, and the velocity pres- sure (Tigerstedt) : H, A reservoir containing water; 1, 2, 3, 4, 5,_ the outflow tube with gauges set at right angles to measure the side pressure; h', the portion of the total pressure used in overcoming the resistance to the flow; h, the portion of the total pressure used in moving the column of liquid — the velocity pressure. coming the resistance along the tube. What is left — that is, H-h', represents the force that is employed in driving the liquid through the tube with a certain velocity ; this portion of the pressure we may speak of as the velocity pressure, h. If in measuring the side pressure at any point the gauge were prolonged into the tube and bent so as to face the stream, (his velocity pressure would add itself to the side pressure at that point and the water would rise to a higher level in this particular tube. There arc two important differences between the circulation as it exists in the body and that repre- sented by the model. In the body, in the first place, we have the area of capillaries, small arteries, and veins, intercalated be- tween the large arteries on one side and the veins on the other; and, in the second place, the vessels, especially the arteries, are extensible and elastic. The effect caused by the first of these BLOOD-PRESSURE AND BLOOD-VELOCITY. 497 factors — namely, a great resistance placed in the middle of the course — may be illustrated by the model shown in Fig. 206, which differs from that in Fig. 205 in having a stopcock in the outflow tube, which, when partly turned off „ makes a narrow opening and a relatively great resistance. When the stopcock is open the pressure falls equally throughout the tube, provided the bore of the stopcock is equal to that of the tube. If, however, it is partially turned the side pressure is much increased between it and the reservoir on what we may term the arterial side of the schema, and it is correspond- ingly diminished between the stopcock and the exit, on the venous side of the schema. Substantially this condition prevails in the body. The capillary region, including the smallest arterioles and veins, offers a great resistance to the flow of blood, and this resistance is spoken of in physiology as the peripheral resistance. Its effect is to 1 -- --. i i ^_^ Fig. 206. — Schema like the preceding except that a stopcock is inserted at the middle of the outflow to imitate the peripheral resistance of the capillary area. The relations of the internal pressure on the arterial and venous sides of this special resistance is shown by the height of the water in the gauges. raise the pressure on the arterial side and lower it on the venous side. When other conditions in the circulation remain constant it is found that an increase in peripheral resistance, caused usually by a con- striction of the arterioles, is followed by a rise of arterial pressures and a fall of venous pressures. On the contrary, a dilatation of the arterioles in any organ is followed by a fall of pressure in its artery or arteries and a rise of pressure in its veins. The effect of the elastic- ity of the arteries is of importance in connection with the fact that in reality the circulation is charged with blood not from a constant reservoir as in the models, Figs. 205 and 206, but by the rhythmical beats of the heart. If the vascular system were perfectly rigid each rhythmical charge into the aorta would be followed by an equal dis- charge from the venae cavae, the pressure throughout the system would rise to a high point during systole and fall to zero during the 32 498 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. diastole. The elasticity of the arteries, in connection with the peripheral resistance, makes an important difference. As the heart discharges into the aorta the pressure rises, but the walls of the arterial system are distended by the increased pressure, and during the following diastole the recoil of these distended walls maintains a flow of blood through the capillaries into the veins. With a certain rapidity of heart beat the distension of the arterial walls is increased to such a point that the outflow through the capillaries into the veins is as great during diastole as during systole; the rhythmical flow in the arteries becomes converted by the elastic tension of the overfilled arterial system into a continuous flow in the capillaries and veins. This effect may be illustrated by a simple schema such as is represented in Fig. 207. A syringe bulb (a), rep- resenting the heart, is connected by a short piece of rubber tubing to a glass tube (b), and also by a piece of distensible band tubing (e) with Fig. 207. — Simple schema to illustrate the factors producing a constant head of pres- sure in the arterial system : a, A syringe bulb with valves, representing the heart ; b, glass tube with fine point representing a path with resistance alone, but no extensibility (the out- flow is in spurts synchronous with tlie strokes of the pump); c, outflow with resistance and also extensible and elastic walls represented by the large rubber bag, e ; the outflow is a steady stream due to the elastic recoil of the distended bag, e. a similar glass tube drawn to a fine point (c). In the latter case the distensible, elastic tubing represents the arterial system, and the fine pointed glass tube the peripheral resistance of the capillary area. If the syringe bulb is put into rhythmical play and the flow is directed through tube b the discharges are in rhythmical spurts, but if directed through tube c the discharge is a continuous stream, since I he force of the separate beats becomes stored as elastic tension in the walls of the band tubing, and it is this constant force which drives a steady stream through the capillary point. In a general way, this schema gives us a true picture of the conditions in the cir- culation. The rhythmical force of the heart beat is stored as elastic iiii ion in the walls of the arteries, and it is the squeeze of these distended walls which gives the continuous driving force that is responsible for- the constant flow in the capillaries and veins. Enumeration of the Factors Concerned in Producing Nor- mal Pressure and Velocity. — In the normal circulation we may BLOOD-PRESSURE AND BLOOD-VELOCITY. 499 say that four chief factors co-operate in producing the conditions of pressure and velocity as we find them. These factors are: (1) The heart beat. (2) The resistance to the flow of blood through the vessels, and especially the peripheral resistance in the region of the small arteries, capillaries, and small veins. (3) The elasticity of the arteries. (4) The quantity of blood in the system. The way in which these factors act may be pictured as follows : Suppose the system at rest with the definite quantity of blood distributed equally throughout the vascular system. The internal or side pressure throughout the system will be everywhere the same, — probably zero (atmospheric) pressure, since the capacity of the vascular system is sufficient to hold the entire quantity of blood without distension of its walls. If, now, the heart begins to beat with a definite rhythm and discharges a definite quantity of blood at each beat the whole mass will be set into motion. The arteries receive the blood more rapidly than it can escape through the capil- laries into the veins, and consequently it accumulates upon the arterial side until an equilibrium is reached, — that is, a point at which the elastic recoil of the whole arterial tree suffices to force through the capillaries in a unit of time as much blood as is received from the heart during the same time. In this condition of equilib- rium the flow in capillaries and veins is constant, and the side pressure in the veins increases from the right auricle back to the capillaries. In the arteries there is a large side pressure throughout, owing to the resistance between them and the veins and especially to the great resistance offered by the narrow capillaries. This pressure rises and falls with each discharge from the heart, and the pulse waves, both as regards pressure and velocity, are most marked in the aorta and diminish farther out in the arterial tree, failing completely in the last small arterioles, since if taken together these arterioles constitute a large and distensible tube of much greater capacity than the aorta. General Conditions Influencing Blood-pressure and Blood- velocity. — Alterations in any of the four chief factors mentioned above must, of course, cause a change in pressure and velocity. I. An increase in the rate or force of the heart beat will increase the velocity of the flow throughout the system, although, of course, that general difference in velocity in the arteries, capillaries, and veins which depends upon the variations in width of bed will remain. Such a change will also cause a rise of pressures throughout the system. The whole energy exhibited in the vascular system as side pressure, velocity pressure, etc., comes, in the long run, from the force of contraction of the heart muscle. This force is what is represented in the model, Fig. 196, as the total head of pressure (H). An increase in rate or force of heart beat is equivalent, therefore, 500 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. to an increase in this head of pressure, and along with the increase in velocity thus caused there is an increased friction or resistance. II. An increase or decrease in the width of the vessels will influence both the resistance to the flow and the velocity. Under normal conditions it is the small arteries that are constricted or dilated (vasoconstriction and vasodilatation). A constriction of these arteries causes an increase in arterial pressures and a decrease in venous pressure. The velocity of the blood-flow is decreased. A dilatation has the opposite effects. Numerous instances of this relation will be referred to in describing the physiology of the vaso- motor nerves. III. A diminution in elasticity of the arteries will tend to interfere with the constancy of the flow from the arteries into the capillaries, and in the arteries themselves the swings of pressure from systolic to diastolic during the heart beat will be more extensive. This latter fact can be shown upon elderly individuals whose arteries are becoming rigid, but whether a change of this character is ever so advanced in human beings as to seriously modify the capillary circulation does not appear to have been investigated. IV. A loss of blood, other conditions remaining the same, will also cause a fall in blood-pressures and velocity. As a matter of fact, however, a considerable amount of blood may be lost with- out any marked permanent change in arterial blood-pressure. The reason for this result is found in the adjustability or adaptability of the vascular system. It is in such respects that the system differs greatly from a rigid schema such as we use for our models. When blood is withdrawn from the vessels the loss may be offset by an increased action of the heart and by a contraction of the arterioles, the two effects combining to give a normal or approximately normal arterial pressure. To carry out the analogy with the model (Fig. 205) if by chance some of the store of water was lost we might sub- stitute a narrower reservoir, so that with a diminished supply we could still maintain the same level of pressure. In the body, moreover, a loss of blood by hemorrhage may be compensated in p.nt. bo far as Hie bulk of the liquid is concerned, by a flow of liquid from the tissues into the blood-vessels. The Hydrostatic Effect. — In the living animal, especially in those, like ourselves, that walk upright, the actual pressure in the arteries of the various tissues must vary much also with the position. For instance, in standing erect the small arteries in the hands or feet are, in addition to other conditions noted above, exposed to the weight of the column of arterial blood standing over them. In the pendent arm the skin of the fingers is congested; if, however, the arm is raised above the head the skin may become blanched because now the column of blood from fingers to shoulder exercises BLOOD-PRESSURE AND BLOOD-VELOCITY. 501 a hydrostatic pressure in the opposite direction. In determinations of blood-pressure in the brachial artery of man care should be taken to keep the arm in the same position in a series of observations in order to equalize the effect of the hydrostatic factor. The impor- tance of this gravity effect is most evident in the case of the ab- dominal (splanchnic) circulation. When an animal accustomed to go on all fours is held in a vertical position the great vascular area of the abdomen is placed under an increased pressure due to gravity, and, unless there is a compensatory contraction of the arterioles or of the abdominal wall, so much blood may accumulate in this portion of the system that the arterial pressure in the aorta will fall markedly or the circulation may stop entirely.* In most cases the compensation takes place and no serious change in the circulation results. In rabbits, however, which have lax abdominal walls, it is said that the animal may be killed by simply holding it in the erect position for some time. For the same reason an erect posture in man may be dangerous when the compensatory nervous reflexes controlling the arteries and the tone of the abdominal wall are thrown out of action, as, for instance, in a faint or in a condition of anesthesia. In such conditions the recumbent position favors the maintenance of the normal circulation. Indeed, under ordinary conditions some individuals are quite sensitive to the effects of a vertical position, especially if unaccompanied by muscular or mental activity, and may suffer from giddiness and a sense of faintness in consequence of a fall in general blood-pressure. It seems prob- able that in these cases the gravity effect has drafted off an undue amount of blood into the splanchnic area. Individuals who have been kept in bed for long periods by sickness, accident, or other causes suffer from giddiness and unsteadiness when they first attempt to stand or walk. It seems quite possible that in such cases the effect is caused by a fall in arterial pressure brought about by the dilatation in the splanchnic area. The added weight of blood thrown on these vessels by the effect of gravity is not compensated by a vasoconstriction of the arterioles or an increased tone in the abdominal walls. While certain general deductions of the kind given above may be made from our knowledge of the hydrodynamics and hydrostatics of the cir- culation, it is evident that in particular cases, whether affecting special organs or the organism as a whole, it is necessary to obtain directly, if possible, the facts, not only for the arterial pressure and velocity but also for the venous pressure and velocity, in order to draw safe conclusions as to the changes in the circulation. In all observations made upon man it is especially important to standardize the results by reducing * Hill and Barnard, "Journal of Physiology," 21, 321, 1897. 502 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. them to a common level. The arterial or venous pressure in the foot or hand of a man standing erect is increased by the hydrostatic effect of the vertical column of blood between the point measured and the heart. This hydrostatic effect varies, of course, for the different parts of the body, and to compare the pressures in the different arteries or veins with one another the vertical distance from the heart should be measured and this pressure in terms of a column of water or mercury should be subtracted or added, as the case may be, to the pressure actually observed. The exact level for which these measurements should be adjusted has varied somewhat in practice; to simply say the heart-level is too indefinite, since in the upright position there is a considerable distance between the level of the base and of the apex of the heart, von Recklinghausen recommends the middle of the dorsoventral axis drawn from the lower end of the sternum to the spinal column. Accessory Factors Aiding the Circulation. — The force of the heart beat is the main factor concerned in the movement of the blood, but certain other muscular movements aid more or less in maintaining the circulation as it actually exists in the living animal. The most important of these accessory factors are the respiratory movements and the contractions of the muscles of the limbs and viscera. At each inspiratory movement the pressure relations are altered in the thorax and abdomen, and reverse changes occur during expiration. These effects influence the flow of blood to the heart, and alter the velocity and pressure of the blood in a way that is described in the section on Respiration under the title of The Respiratory Waves of Blood-pressure. Contractions of the skeletal muscles must also influence the blood-flow. The thickening of the fibers in contraction squeezes upon the capillaries and small vessels and tends to empty them. On account of the valves in the veins the blood is forced mainly toward the venous side of the heart; .so that rhythmical contractions of the muscles may accelerate the circulation. This pumping effect of our muscular movements is probably quite an important factor in returning the blood from the lower extremities. In this portion of the body the venous flow to the heart lias to overcome the hydrostatic pres- sure of the column of blood, and it has been shown that when one is standing quite still the venous pressure alone may be insufficient to overcome this resistance, so that the blood-flow from the feet may be much retarded. Under these circum- stances movements of the legs, us in walking, aided by the valves in the veins, probably help to "milk" the blood toward the heart. The contractions of the smooth muscles, especially in the stomach and intestines, during digestion have a similar BLOOD-PRESSURE AND BLOOD-VELOCITY. 503 effect. The musculature of the spleen also is supposed to aid the circulation through that organ by its rhythmical contractions. The Conditions of Pressure and Velocity in the Pulmonary Circulation. — The general plan of the smaller circulation from right ventricle to left auricle is the same as in the major or systemic circulation, and the same general principles hold. The right ventricle pumps its blood into the pulmonary artery, and, on ac- count of the peripheral resistance in the lung capillaries, the side pressure in the artery is higher than in the capillaries, and higher in these than in the pulmonary veins. The velocity of movement is least, on the other hand, in the extensive capillary area and greatest in the pulmonary artery and veins, on account of the variations in width of the bed. So also in the pulmonary artery the pressure and velocity must fluctuate between a systolic and diastolic level at each heart beat, while in the pulmonary veins they are more or less uni- form. An interesting difference between the two circulations consists in the fact that the peripheral resistance is evidently much less in the pulmonary circuit, and consequently the pressure in the pulmonary arteries is much less than in the aortic system. The velocity of the flow, as already stated (p. 472), is also greater in the lung capillaries than in the systemic capillaries. Exact deter- minations of the pressure in the pulmonary artery are made with difficulty on account of the position of the vessel.* The results obtained by various observers give such values as the following: Mean Pressure. Extreme Variations. Mms. Hg. Mms. Hg. Dog 20 10 to 33 Cat 18 7.5 " 24.7 Rabbit 12 6 "35 It will be seen, therefore, that the mean pressure is not more than one-seventh to one-eighth of that prevailing in the aorta. The thinner walls and smaller muscular power of the right ventricle as compared with the left are an indication of the fact that less force is necessary to keep up the circulation through the pulmonary circuit. The Variations in Pressure in the Pulmonary Circuit. — Experimental results indicate that the pressures in the pulmonary circuit do not undergo as marked changes as in the systemic circu- lation; the flow is characterized by a greater steadiness. With a systemic pressure, as taken in the carotid, varying from 144 to 222 mms., that in the pulmonary artery changes correspondingly only from 20 to 26 mms., and extreme variations of pressure in the * For a discussion of the special physiology of the pulmonary circulation and for references to literature, see Tigerstedt, "Ergebnisse der Physiologie," vol. ii., part ii., p. 528, 1903. 504 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. pulmonary artery probably do not exceed,, as a rule, 15 to 20 mms. The regulations of the pressure and flow of blood in the small circulation do not seem to be so direct or complex as in the aortic system. The part taken by the vasomotor nerves is referred to in the chapter upon the innervation of the blood-vessels, and attention may be called here only to the mechanical factors, which, indeed, for this circulation are probably the most important. The output from the right ventricle, and therefore the amount of flow and the pressure in the pulmonary artery, depends mainly on the amount of blood received through the venoe cavae by the right auricle. If one of the vense cava? is closed the pulmonary pressure sinks; pressure upon the abdomen, on the other hand, by squeezing more blood toward the right heart may raise the pressure in the pulmonary artery. By such means, therefore, the variations in blood-flow in the systemic circulation indirectly influence and control the pressure relations in the pulmonary circuit. But the changes in the systemic circulation may affect the blood-flow through the lungs in still another way, — namely, by a back effect through the left auricle. When for any reason the blood-pressure in the aorta is driven much above the normal level the left ventricle may not be able to empty itself sufficiently, and if this happens the pressure in the left auricle will rise and the flow through the lungs from right ventricle to left auricle will be more or less impeded. On the whole, it would seem that the pulmonary circulation is subject to less changes than in the case of the organs supplied by the aorta. The mechanical conditions, especially in the capillary region, are such that the blood is sent through the lungs with a relatively high velocity, although under small actual pressure. The special effects of the respiratory movements and of variations in intrathoracic pressure upon the pulmonary circulation are referred to in con- nection with respiration. CHAPTER XXVII. THE PULSE. General Statement. — When the ventricular systole discharges a new quantity of blood into the arteries the pressure within these vessels is increased temporarily. If the arteries, capillaries, and veins were perfectly rigid tubes it is evident that this pressure would be transmitted practically instantaneously throughout the system, and that a quantity of blood would be displaced from the venae cavse into the auricles equal to the quantity forced into the aorta by the ventricle. The flow of blood throughout the vascular system would take place in a series of spurts or pulses, the pressure rising suddenly during systole and falling rapidly during diastole. Since the blood is incompressible and the walls of the vessels if rigid would be inextensible, the rise of pressure, the pulse, would be simultaneous in all parts of the system. The fact, however, that the walls of the vessels are extensible and elastic modifies the trans- mission of the pulse wave in several important particulars: It explains why it is that the pulse dies out in or at the beginning of the capillaries and why it occurs at different times in different arteries — that is, why the wave of pressure takes a perceptible time to travel over the arteries. The result that follows from the elasticity of the arteries may be pictured as follows: Under the normal conditions of the circulation when the heart contracts and forces a new quantity of blood into the aorta room must be made for this blood either by moving the whole mass of the blood forward — that is, by discharging an equal amount at the other end into the auricle — or by the enlargement of the arteries. This latter alter- native is what really happens, as it takes less pressure to distend the aorta than to move forward the entire mass of blood under the conditions that exist in the body. So soon, therefore, as the semi- lunar valves open and the new column of blood begins to enter the aorta, the walls of that vessel begin to expand and during the time that the blood is flowing out of the heart — that is, in round numbers, about 0.3 sec. — the extension of the walls passes from point to point along the arterial system. At the end of the outflow from the heart all the arteries are beginning to enlarge, the maximum extension being in the aorta, and room is thus made for the new quantity of blood. The new blood that is actually discharged from the heart 505 506 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. lies somewhere in the aorta, but the pressure that has been trans- mitted along the system and has caused it to expand has made room for the blood forced out of the aorta by the new blood. With the cessation of the heart beat and the closure of the semilunar valves, the sharp recoil of the distended aorta drives forward the column of blood, and as the aorta sinks back to its normal diastolic diameter the more distal portions of the arterial system are at first distended to a certain point and then return to their diastolic size as the excess of blood streams through the capillaries into the veins. At the time that the aorta has reached its diastolic size the walls of the most distant arterioles have passed their maximum extension and are beginning to collapse. The distension caused by the pulse, therefore, spreads through the arterial system in the form of a wave. At any given point the distension of the walls increases to a maxi- mum and then declines, and when this change in size is recorded in the large arteries, by methods described below, it is found that the expansion of the artery is much more sudden than the subse- quent collapse. This difference is understood when we remember that the heart throws its load of blood into the arteries with sud- denness and force, causing a sharp rise of pressure, while the collapse of the arteries is due to their own elasticity. The disappearance of the pulse before reaching the capillary area is readily compre- hended when one remembers that the arterial tree constantly in- creases in size as one passes out from the aortic trunk. Many facts, such as those of pressure and velocity already described, indicate that the increase in capacity of the arterial system is somewhat gradual until the region of the smallest arterioles and capillaries is reached and that at this point there is a sudden widening out or increase in capacity of the whole system, although the individual vessels diminish in diameter. It is in this region that the pulse, under ordinary conditions, becomes imperceptible.* When the arterioles in any organ are dilated the pulse may spread through the capillary regions and be visible in the veins. Velocity of the Pulse Wave. — From the above considerations it is evident that in a system such as is presented by our blood- vessels the velocity of the pulse wave must vary with the rigidity of the tubes. If perfectly rigid the pressure would be transmitted practically instantaneously; if the walls were very extensible the prepagation would be relatively slow. For our blood-vessels as they exist at any given moment the velocity of the pulse wave may be estimated by a simple method: Two arteries may be selected at different distances from the heart and the pulse wave as it passes by * For ;i satisfactory discussion of the pulse and for literature consult, von Prey, "Die Untersuchung des Pulses." Berlin, 1892. For ;i description of the variations in disease consult Mackenzie, " The Study of the Pulse, etc." New York. 1902. THE PULSE. 507 a given point in each artery may be recorded by some convenient apparatus, such as can be devised in any laboratory. If the waves are recorded on a rapidly revolving kymographion whose rate of movement can be determined, then the difference in time in the arrival of the pulse wave at the two points is easily ascertained. That there is a perceptible difference in time one can easily demon- strate to himself by feeling simultaneously the pulse of the radial and the carotid arteries. If this difference in time is determined for two arteries — for instance, the femoral and the tibialis anterior — and the distance between the two points is recorded, we have evidently the necessary data for obtaining the velocity of the pulse wave in the arteries of that region. A record of this kind is shown in Fig. 208. Fig. 308. — To illustrate the method of determining the velocity of the pulse wave in man. Shows record of the pulse at two points on the leg at a known distance apart. The difference in time is given by the verticals dropped from the beginning of these waves to the time curve. This last is made by the vibrations of a tuning fork giving 50 vibra- tions per second. The difference in this case was equal to 0.07 sec. The results obtained by various authors indicate that the velocity varies somewhere between 6 and 9 meters per second for adults. The figures published by recent observers show also that the velocity is somewhat greater in the upper extremities (7.5 m. for carotid- radial estimation) than in the descending aorta (6.5 m. for carotid- femoral estimation).* The average of thirty determinations made in the author's laboratory upon medical students shows that the velocity in the leg (femoral-anterior tibial) is 6.1 m. when the records are made upon the same leg, and 7.4 m. when the record for the femoral is taken from one leg and that for the anterior tibial * Edgren, "Skandinavisches Archiv f. Physiol.," 1, 96, 1889. 508 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. from the other. The latter condition would seem to be more nor- mal, since the blood-flow and normal tension of the walls are probably less disturbed. An increase in rigidity of the arteries causes the velocity to rise; in elderly people, therefore, the velocity is distinctly greater. In arterial sclerosis with hypertrophy of the heart the velocity may increase to as much as 11 or 13 m. Any marked dilatation of the arteries — such as occurs, for instance, in aneurysms, — retards the pulse wave markedly; so that the existence of an aneurysm may be detected in some cases by this fact. If we know the velocity of the wave and the time that it takes to pass any given point the length of the wave is given by the formula 1= rt. In an adult the duration of the wave (t) at the radial may be taken as 0.5 to 0.7 sec; so that if the velocity of the wave were uniform throughout the arteries the length of the wave would be from 3.25 to 4.5 m. We can conclude, therefore, that before Fig. 209. — The Dudgeon sphygrnograph in position. the wave has disappeared at the root of the aorta it has reached the most distant arteries. The Form of the Pulse Wave — Sphygmography. — The pulse wave may be felt upon any superficial artery in consequence of the distension of the vessel. By the tactile sense alone the experienced physician may distinguish some of the characters of the wave, its frequency, its force, etc. The details of the form of the wave, however, were made evident only when the variations in size of the artery were recorded graphically by placing a lever upon it. Any instrument suitable for this purpose is designated as a sphygrno- graph, and very numerous forms have been devised. The move- tnenl of the artery is very small and to obtain a distinct record it is necessary to magnify this movement greatly by a properly con- structed lever. The form of lever thai is perhaps most frequently employed i.s shown in the accompanying figures. The instrumenl is strapped upon fcne arm so that the THE PULSE. 509 button of the metallic spring rests over the radial artery. The movements of the artery are transmitted to this spring and this latter in turn acts upon the bent lever, and the magnified movement is recorded by the writing point, upon a strip of blackened paper which is moved under the point by clockwork contained in the case. To obtain a satisfactory record or sphyg- mogram, two details are of special importance: First, the button of the lever must be pressed upon the artery with the proper force. Theo- retically this pressure should be about equal to the diastolic pressure within the artery. All sphygmographs are provided with means to regulate the pressure, and practically one must learn so to place the button and to arrange the pressure as to obtain the largest tracing. A second detail of importance is that the weight of the lever when set suddenly into motion causes a movement, due to the inertia of the mass, which may alter the true form of the wave. To overcome this defect the lever should be as light as possible, or the spring upon which the artery plays should have considerable resis- tance. In those sphygmographs in which the inertia factor is practically eliminated the diffi- culty of obtaining a tracing, especially from a weak pulse, is correspondingly increased, and in the sphygmographs most commonly employed, such as the Dudgeon, facility in application is obtained at the expense of incomplete correction of the error of inertia. The pulse wave obtained from the radial artery is represented in Fig. 211. It will be seen from this figure that the artery dilates rapidly and then falls more slowly, but it must be borne in mind that the very pointed apex of the wave recorded by this form of sphygmograph is due to the instrumental error referred to above, namely, the "fling" of the lever caused by the sudden expansion Fig. 210.— The lever of the Dudgeon sphygmograph: P, The button of the spring F, to be placed upon the artery. The movement is transmitted to the lever, F\, and thence to the bent lever, F2, whose movement is effected through the weight, g. The writing point S, of this lever makes the record on the smoked sur* face, A. Fig. 211. — Sphygmogram from the radial artery, Dudgeon sphygmograph: D, The dicrotic wave; P, the predicrotic wave. of the artery. The ascending portion of the wave is spoken of as the anacrotic limb, the descending, as the catacrotic limb. Under usual conditions the anacrotic limb is smooth, — that is, shows no secondary waves, — while the catacrotic limb shows one or more secondary waves, which are spoken of in general as the catacrotic waves. The most constant of these latter waves occurs usually 510 CIRCULATION* OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. approximately at the middle of the descent (D) and is designated as the dicrotic wave. A less conspicuous wave between it and the apex of the pulse wave is known usually as the predicrotic wave, P, while the wave or waves following the dicrotic are designated as postdicrotic. These catacrotic waves are too small, under normal conditions, to be felt by the finger. Under certain abnormal conditions, however, which cause a low blood-pressure without marked diminution in the heart beat, the dicrotic wave is empha- sized and may be detected by the finger. A pulse of this kind is known as a dicrotic pulse. In each pulse wave we may distinguish a systolic and a diastolic phase; the former, making due allowance for transmission, corresponds with the time during which the aortic valves are open, and blood is streaming from the heart to the aorta, the latter represents the period during which the aortic valves are closed and the arteries are shut off from the heart. In Fig. 21 1 the systolic phase extends from s to d, the diastolic from d to sf. Explanation of the Catacrotic Waves. — It has been found difficult to give an entirely satisfactory explanation of the catacrotic waves or, to speak more accurately, it is difficult to decide between the different explanations that have been proposed. Concerning the dicrotic wave, it may be said that tracings from different ar- teries show that, like the main pulse wave, it has a centrifugal course, — that is, it starts in the aorta and runs peripherally with the same velocity as the main wave upon which it is superposed. More- over, simultaneous tracings of the pressure changes in the heart and in the aorta show that the closure of the semilunar valves is synchro- nous with the small depression or negative wave (d, Fig. 211) which immediately precedes the dicrotic wave. The general belief, there- fore, is that the dicrotic wave results from the closure of the semi- lunar valves. When the distended aorta begins to contract by virtue of the elasticity of its walls, it drives the column of blood in both directions. Owing to the position of the semilunar valves the flow to the ventricle is prevented; but the interposition of this sudden Mock causes a reflected wave; which passes centrifugally over the arterial system. The dicrotic; wave is preceded by a small negative; wave or notch in the curve which marks the time Of closure or just follows the closure of the semilunar valves. The sequence of events as pictured by Mackenzie * is as Follows: "As soon a- the aortic pressure rises above the ventricular the valves close. At the momenl this happens the valves arc supported by the hard, contracted ventricular walls. The withdrawal of the support by the Sudden relaxation of these walls will tend to produce; a negative pressure wave in the arterial system. But this negative * Mackenzie, "The Study of the Pulse arid the Movements of the Heart," L902. THE PULSE. 511 wave is stopped by the sudden stretching of the aortic valves, which, on losing their firm support, have now themselves to bear the resistance of the arterial pressure. This sudden checking of the negative wave starts a second positive wave, which is prop- agated through the arterial system as the dicrotic wave." The smaller waves, such as the predicrotic, have been explained simply as reflected waves, or as instrumental errors, due to fling of the lever. According to some authors,* an important — perhaps the chief — factor in the production of the secondary waves is the reflection that occurs from the periphery. Where each arterial stem breaks up into its smaller vessels the main pulse wave suffers a reflection, a wave running backward toward the heart. It is probable that such reflected waves from different areas — for instance, from the coronary system, the subclavian system, the mesenteric system, etc. — meet in the aorta and may in part summate to larger waves, which again pass peripher- ally. The catacrotic waves, according to this view, probably differ in character in the different arteries, and tracings indicate that this is the case. The radial pulse differs, for instance, from the carotid pulse in the character of its waves. Between these Fig. 212. — Anacrotic pulse from a case of aortic stenosis (Mackenzie): b. The anacrotic wave. opposite views it is not possible to decide, but it is perhaps permissible to believe that while the dicrotic wave is due pri- marily to the impulse following upon the closure of the semilunar valves, nevertheless the actual form of this and the other second- ary waves is variously modified in different parts of the system by the reflected waves from different peripheral regions. f Anacrotic Waves. — As was said above, the anacrotic limb under normal conditions shows no secondary waves. Under pathological conditions, however, a secondary wave more or less clearly marked may appear, as is shown, for instance, in the tracing given in Fig. 212. Such waves are recorded in cases of stiff arteries or stenosis of the semilunar valves. In the normal individual an anacrotic pulse in the radial may be obtained, according to von Kries/J by raising the arm. He believes that in this position the reflection of the pulse wave from the periph- * See von Frey, loc. cit. f For a general discussion, see Tigerstedt, " Ergebnisse d. Phvsiologie, " vol. viii., 1909. X Von Kries, "Studien zur Pulslehre," 1892. 512 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. ery is favored, and that the anacrotic wave is simply a quickly reflected wave. An opposite interpretation, however, is given by von Recklinghausen, who states that conditions which lead to a diminution in vascular tone and a dilation of the arteries produce "weak reflection" and an anacrotic pulse. Constric- tion of the small arteries in any system favors quick reflection in the artery supplying the system and produces a pulse with a sharp-pointed apex. Characteristics of the Pulse in Health and in Disease. — By mere palpation the physician obtains from the pulse valuable indications concerning the heart and the circulation. The fre- quency of the heart beat is at once made evident, so far at least as the ventricle is concerned. One may determine readily whether the frequency is above or below the normal, whether the rhythm is regular or irregular. By the same means one can determine Fig. 213.— SphyKmoRrama illustrating the n the curve for the auricle and vein I lie wave from a to b represents t he auricular contraction, the a wave; that beginning at // is the. wave due to ventricular systole, the c wave, and the rise of pressure extending from d to e and ending with the opening of the auriculoventricular valves constitutes the v wave. The time relations are given along the ab ci a in tenth- of a second, the pressure relations in nuns, of mercury for the ventricle and aoita are ^iven alonn the ordinal es to the left.. (After [I'rvilcricq.) in the diagram given by Fredericq, which isreproduced in Fig. 216. Following this author,* the series of positive and negative waves which may usually be shown in the auricles and great during a single heart boat may be enumerated as follows: 1. The auricular wave (a wave), auricular systole. 2. The first negative wave, auricular diastole. 3. First systolic wave (positive), c wave. Beginning of ventricular systole. Due to sudden closure and protrusion of the auriculoventricular valves. •Fredericq, "Centralblatl f. Physiol.," 22, No. LO, L908. THE PULSE. 517 4. Second negative wave. At the time of opening of the semi- lunar valves. Due to descent of the base of the ventricle, causing dilatation of auricle. 5. Second systolic wave (positive), v wave. Latter part of systole. Due to gradual filling of auricle and at the end to the return of the base of the ventricle to its diastolic position. 6. Postsystolic (third) negative wave begins at moment of opening of the a-v valves. Due to emptying of auricular blood into ventricle. Other waves have been described, especially one in the post- systolic or early diastolic phase of the ventricular beat, which is known as the h wave (Hirschf elder) or b wave (Gibson). This wave occurs between the v and the a wave, it is seen only occasionally in the tracings, and has been referred to the tri- cuspid valves, which at this time are thrown suddenly into position as the ventricle is distended by the inflow of venous blood in early diastole. For the variations in the form of the venous pulse under pathological conditions of the heart, reference must be made to clinical literature.* * See Hewlett, "Journal of Medical Research," 17, 1907; "Journal of the Amer. Med. Assoc," 51, 1908, and "International Clinics," 4, 1907. CHAPTER XXVIII. THE HEART BEAT. General Statement. — We divide the heart into four chambers, — the two auricles and the two ventricles. What we designate as a heart beat begins with the simultaneous contraction of the two auricles, immediately followed by the simultaneous contraction of the two ventricles; then there is a pause, during which the whole heart is at rest and is filling with blood. As a matter of fact, the heart beat is initiated, according to usual belief, not by the auricles proper, but by a contraction of the mouths of the large veins — venae cava1 — where they open into the auricle. This portion of these veins corresponds physiologically to a definite chamber, the venous I?i7li Mill BUI !■>«■■■■ IBIJW ■«■■ =22fla»H»H«S2HHfc<«HS532JUMM Sails !S8! mm SSiS! «sai aiiaNaanvan mami ibpmbi una SSSI iSSSESS! ISSSsSiEBSaS iSSS Fig. 217.- — To show the time relations of the auricular systole and diastole, and ven- tricular systole and diastole (Marey) : Or. D, Tracing from right auricle ; Vent. D, tracing from right ventricle; Vent. G, tracing from left ventricle. Obtained from the heart of the horse by means of tubes communicating with the cavities. anus, in the heart of the lower vertebrates. In the description of the heart beat the contraction of the veins is usually neglected, although in a fundamental consideration of the cause of the normal sequence il is of great importance. The contraction of any part of the heart is designated as its systole, its relaxation and period of rest as its diastole. In the heart beat we have, therefore, the auricular systole, the ventricular systole, and the heart pause during which both chambers are in diastole. The general relations of systole, diastole, .•mf systole; k-c, systolic plateau. — (After Marey.) and very rapid. To register them accurately the recording instru- ment must respond with great promptness and at the same time must be free from inertia movements. A mercury manometer, for instance, would be entirely useless For such a purpose, since the heavy mass of mercury could not follow accurately the quick changes in pressure. The recording manometer devised by Hurthle (p. 180) seems to have met the requirements more satisfactorily than other of the numerous instruments described. A typical curve obtained by means of the Hurthle manometer is given in Fig. 222, V. (Consull also the classical curve obtained byChauveaii .and Marey from the hear! of the horse [Tig. 217].) [twill be seen that the ure in the bearl rises suddenly with the beginning of the ven- tricular contraction and a certain time elapses before this pressure THE HEART BEAT. 527 is strong enough to open the semilunar valves. The moment that this occurs (1, on the ventricular curve in Fig. 222) is determined by simultaneous measurement of the pressure in the aorta, it being evident that the pressure will begin to rise in this latter vessel the moment that the valves open. It is interesting to find that the yielding of the valves to the rising pressure in the ventricle is not indicated on the curve itself by any variation, — a fact which indicates that the valves open smoothly, and are not thrown back with a sudden shock. A very characteristic feature of the ventric- ular curve is its flat top, or 'plateau as it is called. In some cases the plateau slopes more or less upward, in other cases downward, depending, doubtless, on the respective values of the force of the Fig. 222. — Synchronous record of the intraventricular pressure (V), and the aortic pressure (A) : S, The time record, — each vibration = ifc sec. ; 0-5, corresponding ordi- nates in the two curves; 1 marks the opening of the semilunar valves; 3 (or shortly after) marks the closure of these valves and the beginning of diastole. — {Hiirthle.) heart contraction and the aortic tension, for during the whole time of the plateau the semilunar valves are open and the ven- tricle is discharging a column of blood into the aorta. The different features of the ventricular systole as gathered from these pressure curves are expressed by Hiirthle * as follows : I. Systole, phase of contraction of the muscle fibers (0 to 3 in Fig. 222, V). (a) Period of tension (0 to 1), during which the auriculo-ventricular and semilunar valves are both closed and the heart muscle is squeezing upon the contained blood. This period ends at the opening of the semilunar valves. (b) Period of emptying (1 to 3). During this time the heart is empty- ing itself into the aorta and the intraventricular pressure remains above aortic pressure. It ends with the cessation of the contrac- tion of the muscle and the beginning of tho rapid relaxation. II. Diastole, phase of relaxation and rest of the muscle fibers. (a) Period of relaxation from 3 until the curve reaches a horizontal. At the beginning of the relaxation the semilunar valves are closed, and from comparison with the aortic curve the instant of the occur- rence of this closure is placed shortly after 3. * Hurthle, "Archiv f. d. gesammte Physiologie, " 49, 84, 1891. 528 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AXD LYMPH. (b) Period of filling. This period begins as soon as the auriculo-ventric- ular valves open and the stream of blood, which had been flowing into the auricle throughout the ventricular systole, is permitted to enter the ventricle. During this period of filling the ventricular pressure rises slightly as the heart becomes turgid with blood. This increase of pressure is indicated in most cardiograms by a gradual rise of the curve during this period. It is shown in the curve of Chauveau and Mary, given in Fig. 217. The Volume Curve and the Ventricular Output. — In the lower animals the thorax may be opened with suitable pre- cautions as regards anesthesia and artificial respiration, and the heart may be placed within a plethysmograph (see p. 584) to measure its changes in volume during systole and diastole. If the whole heart is treated in this way the curve of volume changes is complicated by the fact that one chamber, the auricle, is filling, while the other, the ventricle, is emptying. Fijr. '22'.',.- Diagram to show the arrangement of the Henderson cardiometer. The recording tambour is inverted, so that the systole will give an up-stroke on the curve. (After Hirschfelder.) A more useful disposition of the apparatus is to enclose only the ventricles. Several different forms of plethysmograph have been devised for this purpose, and they are usually spoken of as ctirtiiniiK i< I-.-. The form described by Henderson* is simple and easily applied to the heart. Its structure and the connections of the recording apparatus are indicated in the diagram given in Fi»-. 223. The apparatus consists of a rubber ball with a circular opening at one point. Over this opening is placed a membrane of rubber dam with a central opening through which the heart is introduced as showD in the diagram. The rubber membrane lie- -miidy in the auriculoventricular groove, making an air-tigh1 joint. The interior of the ball is connected by stiff tubing with a recording tambour. By an arrangement of this kind the ventricles are kept, within an air-chamber closed everywhere except at the outlet to the recording tambour. Every change in volume of the ventricles will be recorded accu- ► Henderson, "American Journal of Physiology," L6, 325, L906, and 23, 345, L909, contain also the literature. THE HEART BEAT. 529 r ately provided there is no leak. Moreover, these volume changes may be given absolute values in cubic centimeters if the appa- ratus is calibrated beforehand. The cardiometer furnishes a convenient method of estimating directly the amount of blood entering and leaving the ventricles under varying conditions, as well as the changes in heart-volume that may result from variations in tonicity. When the heart is beating slowly the volume curve has the form shown in Fig. 224. During systole the ventricles shrink in size as the blood is discharged into the aorta and pulmonary artery — the up-stroke of the curve. At the end of the systole, after the closure of the semilunar and the opening of the auriculoventricular valves, the ventricles are i Fig. 224. — Diagram of the normal volume curve (plethysniogram) of the dog's heart when beating at a slow rate (after Hirschf elder). The up-stroke represents the systole, the down-stroke the diastole; 4 to 5 the period of diastasis (Henderson). At 5 the auricular contraction causes a slight additional dilatation of the ventricle. 1, 2, and 3 represent the time of occurrence of the first, second, and third heart-sounds respectively. dilated rapidly by the inflow of venous blood. Henderson has emphasized the fact that the filling takes place nearly as rapidly as the emptying, owing doubtless to the fact that at the end of ventricular systole the auricles are dilated under some pressure, so that their contents escape at once into the ventricles as soon as the intervening valves are opened. The diastolic curve comes back nearly to the base line and then forms a shoulder (4) from which it runs parallel to or approaches gradually to the base line up to the moment of auricular contraction (5). The period of rest of the filled or nearly filled ventricles, which on the curve is shown from 4 to 5, is called the period of diastasis by Henderson. The heart cycle, so far as the ventricles are concerned, falls, therefore, into three periods: 1, Systole: 2. diastole; 3, diastasis. Variations in heart rate affect chiefly the latter period; this becomes shorter and shorter the more rapid the rate. When the heart rate is so rapid that the period of diastasis drops out altogether and the systole begins as soon 34 530 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. as the diastole is complete, then we should have the maximum output of blood per minute. An increase of rate beyond this point would lead to a curtailment of the period of diastole and eventually to a diminished output of blood per minute. Accord- ing to the account just given, the filling of the ventricle is practically completed before the auricles contract. Henderson believes that the contraction of the auricles adds very little or nothing to the change of blood in the ventricles, but other authors, using the same methods, differ from him in this conclusion. It is at least certain that the ventricles are for the most part filled before the auricular contraction comes on — this latter act may add a greater or less amount to this charge, according to the conditions prevailing, and in all cases its contraction, besides initiating the ventricular systole, doubtless serves, by raising the tension in the ventricular chamber, to bring the auriculoventricular valves more completely into the position of closure. When these valves are deficient, as in mitral stenosis, the contraction of the auricles plays a larger part in completing the filling of the ventricles (Hirschf elder). For the cases in which it can be applied, the volume curve enables us to estimate the ventricular discharge at each beat and the outflow per minute. The curve as registered gives the outflow for the two ventricles, one-half of its indicated volume will give the outflow from the left ventricle, and this figure, multiplied by the pulse rate, will give the output per minute. It was formerly assumed that at each systole the ventricles emptied themselves com- pletely, but work of the kind described in this paragraph in which the volume curves were obtained have shown, on the contrary, that at the end of systole a considerable proportion of the blood may be left in the cavity of the ventricle. The amount thus left behind will vary with the rate and other conditions. According to Henderson's figures for the dog, about one-third or somewhat less of the ventricular charge is left in the heart after systole, when the heart is beating at the normal rate (90), and the quantity of blood discharged from the left ventricle at each Bystole is approximately .002 of the body weight. It is evident thai when the aortic pressure rises to an abnormal level the discharge of blood from the left ventricle will be or may be diminished, with the result that the blood barks up in I he left auricle, thus raising the venous pressure in the lungs and retard- ing the pulmonary circulation. On the other hand, as Hender- son has especially emphasized, the outflow from the ventricle musl be Influenced very directly by the inflow into the auricle from the veins. Variations in the size of the blood-vessels, such ilatation of the small arteries or possibly loss of tone in the THE HEART BEAT. 531 veins, may bring about a condition of venous stasis and cut down the supply of blood to the heart on the venous side. Con- siderations of this kind are helpful or necessary in explaining the changes in circulation which occur under pathological conditions. The Heart Sounds. — An interesting and important feature of the heart beat is the occurrence of the heart sounds. Two sounds are usually described, one at the beginning, the other at the end, of the ventricular systole. The first sound has a deeper pitch and is longer than the second, and their relative pitch and duration are represented frequently by the syllables lubb-dup. According to Haycraft,f both tones, from a musical standpoint, fall in the bass clef, and are separated by a musi- cal interval of a minor third. The sounds are readily heard by applying the ear to the thorax over the heart, but for diagnostic purposes the stethoscope is usually employed, and this method of investigation by hearing is designated as auscultation. The importance of these heart sounds in diagnosis was first em- phasized by Laennec (1819), and since his time a great number of theories have been proposed to explain their causation. Indeed, the subject is not yet closed, although certain general views regard- ing their cause and the time of their occurrence are generally accepted. The second sound is found to follow immediately upon the closure of the semilunar valves. The usual view, therefore, is that the sound is due ultimately to the vibrations set up in these valves by their sudden closure. These vibrations are transmitted to the column of blood in the aorta (or pulmonary artery) and then to the intervening tissue of the chest wall. This view is made probable by a number of experimental results, some of the most important of which were brought out by Williams in a report (1836) of a committee appointed by the British Association for the special purpose of investigating the subject. It has been shown: (1) That the second sound disappears before the first sound when the animal is bled to death, and indeed as soon as the heart ceases to throw out a supply of blood sufficient to maintain aortic tension. It disappears also when cuts are made in the ventricles so that the blood may escape otherwise than through the arteries. (2) When the valves of the pulmonary artery and aorta are hooked back in the living animal the second sound is replaced by a murmur due to the rushing back of the blood into the ventricle, and if the valves are dropped back into place the normal second sound is again heard. (3) Similar sounds may be produced if the root of the aorta with its valves in place is excised and attached to a glass tube carrying a column of water. With such an arrangement, if the valves are held * "Journal of Physiology," 11, 486, 1890. 532 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. open for a moment and then closed sharply by the pressure of the column of water a sound similar to that of the second heart sound is heard. The physician uses this view of the cause of the second sound in auscultation, and it is evident that the nature of the sound or its replacement by murmurs will give useful testimony regarding the condition of the semilunar valves. The first heart sound has of- fered more difficulty. It occurs at or shortly before the closure of the auriculo-ventricular valves, and it would seem natural, therefore, to attribute it to the vibration of these valves when suddenly put under tension by the ventricular systole. Most authors, indeed, believe that this factor is at least partially responsible for the sound, — that is, that the sound contains a valvular element. But that this is not the sole cause is shown by the fact that the bloodless beating heart still gives a sound at the time of the ventricular systole. Indeed, if the apex of the rabbit's heart is cut off, it continues to beat for a few minutes and during this time gives a first heart Fig. 225 — To show the time relation of the heart sounds to the ventricular beat (Marey) : Y.D., Tracing of the ventricular pressure in the right ventricle of the horse. Be- low the two marks show, respectively, the time of the first and second sounds. The first occurs immediately after the beginning of systole, the second immediately after the begin- ning of diastole. sound. It is usually said, therefore, that the first heart sound is caused by the combination of at least two factors, — a valvular element due to the vibration of the auriculo-ventricular valves, and a muscular element due to the vibration of the contracting muscular mass. Accepting this view, there is a further difficulty in explain- ing the origin of the muscular element. According to some, it is due to the fact that the contraction of the muscle fibers is not simultaneous throughout (lie ventricle and the friction of the inter- lacing fibers sets up vibration in the muscular mass; according to others, the so-called muscular element is mainly a resonance tone of theear membrane of the auscultator, the shock of the contracting heart sets the tympanic membrane to vibrating. If seems useless to attempt a detailed discussion of these conflicting views, since no convincing statements can be made. Practically, the time at which the heart sounds occur is of great importance. A number of observers have recorded the time upon a cardiography tracing of THE HEART BEAT. 533 the heart beat with results such as are shown in Fig. 225. The figure shows clearly the general fact that the first sound is heard very shortly after the beginning of systole and the second one immediately after the end of systole. The first sound is therefore systolic, and the second sound diastolic. A more exact and de- tailed study of the time relations of the heart sounds has been made by Einthoven and Geluk.* These authors obtained graphic records of the heart sounds. The sounds received first b}^ a microphone were transmitted to a capillary electrometer and the movements of the latter were photographed. As one result of their work they give the schema shown in Fig. 226. It will be seen from this figure that the first sound begins about 0.01 sec. before the cardiogram shows the commencement of systole, and that for the first 0.06 sec. the sound is heard only over the apex of the heart (a-b). Over the oisee. Fig. 226. — Schematic representation of the relation of the heart sounds to the ventric- ular beat: C, The cardiogram; 1, to show the duration of the first heart sound; 2, the duration of the second heart sound; S, the time record, each division corresponding to 0.02 sec. In 1, a-a' marks the instant that the first heart sound is heard over the apex, and b-b' the moment that it is heard at the second intercostal space. — (Einthoven and Geluk.) base of the heart (second intercostal space) the first sound is heard (b to c-d) just at the time when the semilunar valves are opened (&'), — that is, at the beginning of the period of emptying according to the classification given on p. 527. The first sound ceases long before the ventricular contraction itself is over, — a fact which would seem to indicate that the muscular element in the first sound is not a muscular sound, such as is given out by a contracting skeletal muscle. The beginning of the second sound seems to mark exactly the time of closure of the semilunar valves. The character and the time relations of the murmurs that accompany or replace the heart sounds form the interesting practical continuation of this theme; but the subject is so large that the student must be referred for this information to the works upon clinical methods. The Third Heart Sound. — Several observers* have called attention to the fact that in certain individuals a third heart * Einthoven and Geluk, "Archiv f. d. gesammte Physiologie," 57, 617, 1894. Einthoven, ibid., 1907, vol. 117. t Thayer, "Boston Med. and Sure;. Journal," 158, 713, 190S; Einthoven, "Archiv f. d. ges. Physiol.," 120, 31, 1907; Gibson, "Lancet," 1907, II., 1380. 534 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. sound may be heard very shortly (0.13 sec.) after the beginning of the second sound. Thayer describes this sound as being "softer and of lower pitch" than the second sound, and in some cases as resembling rather a dull thud or hum. In those persons in whom it can be detected it is heard most distinctly over the apex of the heart. Einthoven has shown the existence of this sound by objective methods. By means of a microphone attachment the heart sounds can be transmitted to the string- galvanometer, in which they cause deflections of the string that can be photographed. In this way he has obtained records of the third sound upon individuals in whom the stethoscope failed to reveal its existence. The cause of this sound has been explained differently by the several authors who have inves- tigated. It occurs early in the diastole, and Einthoven suggests that it is due to an after-vibration of the semilunar valves. Thayer and Gibson suggest the more probable explanation that it is due to a vibration of the auriculoventricular valves which is set up by the sudden inrush of blood from the auricles at the beginning of diastole. This inflow of venous blood distends the ventricle sharply and throws the valves into a position of closure with some suddenness. The sound occurs at about the time of the shoulder on the diastolic limb of the volume curve, as is indicated in the diagram in Fig. 224. The Events that Occur During a Single Cardiac Cycle. — By a complete cardiac cycle is meant the time from any given feature of the heart beat until that feature is again produced. It may be helpful to summarize the events in such a cycle, both as regards the hearl and as regards the blood contained in it. We may begin with the closure of the semilunar valves. At that moment the second heart sound is heard and at that moment the ventricle is quickly relaxing from its previous contraction. Since the auriculoventricular valves are still closed (see diagram, Fig. 216), the ventricles for a brief interval are shut off on both sides. The blood is flowing steadily into the auricles and dilating them. As soon as the ventricles relax the pressure of blood in the auricles opens the auriculoven- tricular valves, and from thai moment until the beginning of the auricular systole the blood from the large veins is filling both ventricles and auricles. As staled on |». 529, the venous blood which has been accumulating in the auricles during the ventricular aystole flows into the ventricles with some sudden- ne "ii the opening of the auriculoventricular valves. The ventricles, therefore, dilate rapidly and the auriculoventricular valves are floated into a position ready for closure;. This event occur- at about the time that the third heart sound is heard. THE HEART BEAT. 535 In a slowly beating heart there may be quite an interval (period of diastasis) between this point and the auricular contraction. The auricular systole sends a sudden wave of blood into the ventricles, dilating them still further and momentarily blocking or retarding the flow from the large veins, and causing one of the waves seen in the normal venous pulse as recorded in the jugular veins. The ventricular systole follows at once upon the auricular systole, the exact relations in this case depend- ing somewhat upon the pulse rate. As the ventricle enters into contraction the auriculo-ventricular valves are tightly closed, the first sound is heard, and for a short interval the ventricular cavity is again shut off on both sides. Soon the rising pressure in the interior forces open the semilunar valves, and then a column of blood is discharged into the aorta and pulmonary artery as long as the contraction lasts. During this interval the flow at the venous end of the heart continues, the blood being received into the yielding auricles. Indeed, this capacity for receiving the venous inflow during the comparatively long-lasting ventricular systole may be considered as one valuable mechanical function' fulfilled by the auricles. The venous flow is never completely blocked and at the most suffers only a slight retardation during the very brief auricular systole. At the end of the ventricular sys- tole the excess of pressure in the aorta and the pulmonary artery closes the semilunar valves and completes the cycle. Time Relations of Systole and Diastole. — The duration of the separate phases of the heart beat depends natural^ on the rate of beat. Assuming a low pulse rate of 70 per minute, the average duration of the different phases may be estimated as follows: Ventricular systole = 0.379 sec. Ventricular diastole and pause = 0.483 " Auricular systole =0.1 to 0.17 " Auricular diastole and pause = 0.762 to 0.692 " Einthoven and Geluk, in the investigation referred to above, measured the time intervals of systole and diastole during fifteen heart periods of a healthy man, and found that the time for the ventricular systole varied between 0.312 and 0.346 sec, while that for the diastole varied from 0.385 to 0.518 sec. Experiments by a number of observers indicate that in the great changes of rate which the heart may undergo under normal conditions the diastolic phase (period of diastasis) is affected relatively much more than the systolic, as we should expect. The Normal Capacity of the Ventricles and the Work Done by the Heart. — Various efforts have been made to measure the normal capacity of the ventricles in man, but the deter- mination has encountered many difficulties. Experiments and 536 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. observations made upon the excised heart are of little value, since the distensible walls of the ventricles yield readily to pressure, and it is difficult or impossible to imitate exactly the conditions of pressure that prevail during life. Nor is it certain whether normally the ventricles empty themselves completely during systole; in fact, the evidence from experiments on the lower animals indicates that, contrary to the opinion which for- merly prevailed, the ventricles throw out only a portion of their blood at each beat. The older observers (Volkmann, Vierordt) attempted to arrive at a determination of the normal output of the ventricles by calculations based upon the velocity of the blood in the carotid and the width of the stream bed. from observations on many animals they arrived at the general- ization that at each systole the amount of blood ejected from the ventricles is equal to about 4-iro of the body weight. For a man weighing, say, 72 kilograms (158 lbs.) this ratio would give an output for each systole of 180 gms. (6 ozs.). More recent observers, however, have found this estimate too high. Howell and Donaldson* measured the output directly for the heart of the dog, making use of a heart isolated from the body and kept beating by an artificial circulation. The ratio of the output varied with the rate of beat; for a rate of 180 beats per minute it was equal to 0.00117 (-sibr) °f the body weight; for a rate of 120 beats per minute it was equal to 0.0014 (-7-5-0). This ratio is therefore about one-half of that proposed by Volkmann. Tigerstedt, from observations upon rabbits, obtained a lower ratio still (0.00042); but from his own results and those obtained by other workers he concludes f that an average valuation for the volume of blood discharged by each ventricle of the human heart is from 50 to 100 c.c. On this basis one may make an approximate estimate of the work done at each beat. Using Tigerstedt's figures, such results as the follow- ing are obtained: On the left side the heart empties its 100 c.c. against a pressure of 150 mms. Ilg. (0.150 meter) and on the right Bide against a pressure of, say, 60 mms. Hg. (0.06 meter). The work done is calculated from the formula w=pr, in which p repre- Bents the weight of the mass thrown out and r the resistance or mean aortic pressure. This latter factor must be multiplied by 13.6, the density of mercury, to reduce to a column of blood. Left ventricle, 100 gms. X (0.150 x 13.0) = 204.0 grammeters. Right " 100 " X (0.00 X 13.G) = 81.0 l's.i.Ci grammeters. * Bowel! and Donaldson, " Philosophical Transactions," Royal ftoc, Lon- don, 1884. f Tigerstedt, "Lehrbuch der Physiologic des Kreislaufes," p. 152, LS93. THE HEART BEAT. 537 To this must be added the energy represented by the velocity of the mass ejected into the aorta. Placing this velocity at 500 mms. (0.5 meter) for both aorta and pulmonary artery, the energy represented in mechanical work is estimated from the formula ^ in which p represents the weight of the mass moved, v the velocity of its movement, and g the accelerating force of gravity. Applying this formula we have for each ventricle 2 x 9 8' = 1 .28 grammeters, or for both ventricles 2.56 grammeters, making a total of over 288 grammeters of work. That is, the mechanical work done at each contraction of the heart is equal to that necessary to raise 288 gms. a meter in height. The calculations made by different authors as to the amount of blood discharged from each ventricle during systole may be tabulated as follows: Thomas Young 45 gms. Volkmann 188 " for weight of 72 kgms. Vierordt 180 " " " " " " Fick 50-73 " Howell and Donaldson 75-90 " " " "65 " Hoorweg 47 " Zuntz 60 " Tigerstedt 50-100 " Plumier 70 " Loewy and v. Schrotter 55 " " " 60-65 kgms. The Coronary Circulation during the Heart Beat.— The condition of the blood-flow in the coronary vessels during the phases of the heart beat has been the subject of much speculation and experiment, since it has entered as a factor in the discussion of several mechanical and nutritive problems that are connected with the physiology of the heart. According to a view usually attributed to Thebesius (1708), the flaps of the semilunar valves are thrown back during systole and shut off the coronary circulation, and therefore the coronary vessels, unlike those of other organs, are filled during diastole. In modern times this view has been revived by Briicke, who made it a part of his theory of the "self-regulation" of the heart beat. According to this view, the coronaries are shut off from the aorta during systole by the flaps of the semilunar valves, so that the contraction of the ventricle is not opposed by the distended arteries, while, on the other hand, the reinjection of these vessels from the aorta during diastole aids in the dilatation of the ventricular cavities. Experimental work has shown decisively that the part of this theory relating to the closure of the coronary arteries by the semilunar valves is incorrect.* Records of pressure changes in the coronary arteries during the heart beat made by Martin and Sedgwick and by Porter show that they are substantially identical * See Porter, "American Journal of Physiology/' 1, 145, 189S, for dis- cussion and literature. 53S CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. with those in the carotid or aorta, and records of the velocity of the blood-flow made by Rebatel show that at the beginning of systole the flow in the coronaries suffers a sudden systolic acceleration as in the case of other arteries. During systole, therefore, the mouths of the coronary arteries are in free communication with the aorta. But the coronary system — arteries, capillaries, and veins — is in part imbedded in the musculature of the ventricles, and we should suppose that the great pressure exerted by the contracting muscu- lature would at the height of systole clamp off this system and stop the coronary circulation. That this result really happens is indi- dicated by Rebatel's curves of the velocity of the flow in the coro- nary arteries. As shown in Fig. 227, the great acceleration (a) in velocity at the beginning of systole is quickly followed by a drop to zero (b) or even a negative value, — that is, a flow in the other direc- tion, toward the aorta. At the end of the first (relaxation) phase of diastole there is again a sudden increase in velocity (c), corre- sponding with the injection of the arteries from the aorta, followed again by a decrease at the end of the diastole at the time when the ventricular cavity is filled with venous blood under some pressure. Porter, moreover, has shown in an interesting series of experiments that when a piece of the ventricle is kept beating, by supplying it with blood through its nutrient artery from a reservoir at con- stant pressure, each systole causes a jet of blood from the sev- Fig. 227. — Simultaneous record of the blood-pressure (A) and the Wood-velocity (li) in the coronary arterie (Chauveau and Rebatel): a, Murks the beginning of the systole (there i i ri i • m \>n- un- :unl in velocity); 6, marks a second rise of pressure (A) due to the closure of the coronary capillaries by the contracting ventricle (at this moment ui B iii. falls olT rapidly); c, curve (li) shows an increase in velocity due to the open- ing of the mall coronary vessels at the beginning of diastole. ered vessels at the margin of the piece. In fact, the rhythmical squeeze of its own vessels during systole accelerates effectively the coronary circulation. The volume of blood (lowing through the heart vessels increases with the frequency or the force of the beat, since each systole empties the coronary system more or less com- pletely toward the venous side and at each diastole the distended aorta quickly fills the empty vessel. THE HEART BEAT. 539 The Suction-pump Action of the Heart. — So far in con- sidering the mechanics of the circulation attention has been directed only to the force-pump action of the heart. All of the energy of the circulation, the velocity of the flow and the internal pressure, has been referred to the force of contraction of the ventricles as the main cause, and to certain accessory factors, such as the respiratory movements and the contractions of the skeletal muscles, as subsid- iary causes. It is possible, however, that the heart may also act as a suction-pump, sucking in blood from the venous side "in conse- quence of an active dilatation. According to this view, the heart works after the manner of a syringe bulb, which when squeezed forces out liquid from one end and when relaxed sucks it in from the other in consequence of its elastic dilatation. While this view has long been entertained, modern interest in it was aroused chiefly perhaps by the experiments of Goltz and Gaule, which showed that at some point in the heart beat there is or may be a strong negative pressure in the interior of the ventricles.* Their method consisted in connecting a manometer with the interior of the ventricle and interposing between the two a valve that opened only toward the heart. The manometer was thus converted into a minimum manometer, which registered the lowest pressure reached during the period of observation. By this method they and others have shown that in an animal (dog) with an opened thorax the pressure in the interior of the ventricles may be negative to an extent equal to 20, 30, or even 50 mms. of mercury. Moreover, by the use of some form of elastic manometer, such as the Hiirthle instrument (p. 462), it has been shown that this negative pressure occurs at the end of the period of relaxation, at the time, therefore, at which it might be supposed to exert a marked influence upon the inflow of venous blood. It should be added, however, that a negative pressure can not be shown for every heart beat. It may be absent altogether or slight in amount, varying, no doubt, with the force of contraction and the condition of the heart. Physiologists have attempted to determine the cause of this negative pressure and the extent of its influence on the blood-flow. With regard to the first question, so many answers have been proposed that it is difficult to arrive at a satisfactory opinion. According to some, the heart tends to dilate at the end of its systole by virtue of its own elasticity, — that is, the elasticity of its own musculature or of the connective tissue contained in its substance, for example, beneath the en- docardium, in the walls of the arteries, etc. This view, however, finds little or no support from direct experiments made upon the * For a complete discussion of this subject and the literature see the ar- ticle by Ebstein, " Die Diastole des Herzens," in the " Ergebnisse der Physi- ologie," vol. iii, part n, 1904. 540 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. fresh, living heart. If such a heart in a bloodless condition is squeezed by hand there is no evidence of an elastic recoil as in the case of a syringe bulb. Others have explained the negative pressure as due not to a simple elastic expansion, but to what may be called a physiological expansion, — that is, an expansion due to physiological processes, such as anabolic changes. Such a view, however, is at present more or less speculative and can not be con- clusively demonstrated. Still others have traced the expansion of the ventricle and the resulting negative pressure to the sudden in- jection of the coronary system from the aorta at the beginning of diastole. The heart in contracting exerts a force greater than that of the blood in the coronary vessels, and probably, therefore, these vessels are emptied and their cavities obliterated in part. At the beginning of diastole they are reinjected with blood under a pressure of perhaps 100 mms. of mercury, and this fact seems to offer a probable explanation for a partial dilatation of the ventricular cavity and a production of negative pressure in the brief interval before the opening of the auriculo-ventricular valves. No view, however, has met with general acceptance, and the cause or causes that produce the negative intraventricular pressure are still a subject for investiga- tion. Regarding the second question proposed above, — namely, the extent of the influence of this negative pressure on the flow of venous blood to the ventricles, — much diversity of opinion also exists. Direct experiments made by Martin and Donaldson* indicate that this factor has little or no actual influence upon the venous flow. These authors used an isolated dog's heart kept beating by an artificial supply of blood. At a given moment the stream of blood into the vena cava was shut off and the auricle of the heart was brought into communication with a U tube filled with blood. It was found that the auricle took blood from this tube only so long as the pressure in it was positive. Although the heart continued to beat vigorously, whatever negative pressure was present in the ventricle was unable to suck any blood into the auricle from the U tube. Porter f also has shown that at the time of a strong negative pressure in the ventricle the auricle may give little or no evidence of a similar fall in pressure. It would seem most probable, therefore, that the negative pressure observed under certain conditions in the ventricles is a fleeting phenomenon, and disappears with the entrance of the first portion of the blood from the auricles. While it may bo of value in accelerating the opening of the auriculo-venl ricular valves, ils influence does not extend to an * Martin and Donaldson, "Studies from the Biological Laboratory, Johns Hopkins University," 1, 37, L887; also Martin's 'TPhysioloKical Papers," Baltimore, 1895. Sec also, for confirmatory results, von den Velden, "Zeit- Bchrifl f. exp. Pathol, u. Therapie, 1906, iii., 432. Journal of Physiology,'' 13, 513, 1892. THE HEART BEAT. 541 actual suction of the blood from the veins toward the heart. Other authors, however, on theoretical grounds attribute more actual im- portance to the negative pressure as a factor in moving the blood. Occlusion of the Coronary Vessels. — The coronary vessels supply the tissues of the heart with nutrition, including oxygen, so that if the circulation is interrupted the normal contractions soon cease. The branches of the large coronaries form what are known as terminal arteries, — that is, each supplies a separate region of the musculature, and although anastomoses may exist they appear to be too incomplete to allow a collateral circulation to be estab- lished when one of the main arteries is occluded. The portion of the heart supplied by it dies, or to use the pathological term, under- goes necrosis. On account of the pathological interests involved — the known serious results that may follow occlusion of any of the coro- nary Vessels or even any interference with the normal structure of the vessels — a number of investigations have been made upon animals to determine the effect of occluding one or more of the coronary vessels.* It would seem from Porter's experiments that the results of such an operation vary according to the size of the area deprived of its blood. When the arteria septi alone was occluded the heart was not affected, when the arteria coronaria dextra was occluded the ventricular contractions were arrested in 18 per cent, of the cases observed. Occlusion of the ramus descendens of the left coronary artery caused arrest of the ventricles in 50 per cent, of the cases, while occlusion of the circumflex branch of the same artery caused arrest in 80 per cent, of the cases. Ligation of three of the arteries caused stoppage of the heart in all cases. Fibrillar Contractions. — The arrest of the ventricles in the experiments just described followed immediately or within a short period, and the ventricles went into fibrillar contractions. In this curious condition the various fibers of the ventricular muscle, in- stead of contracting together in a co-ordinated fashion, contract separately and irregularly; so that the surface of the ventricle has the appearance of a vibrating, twitching mass. Such a condition in the ventricle is usually fatal, — that is, the musculature is not able to recover its co-ordinated movement. This condition may come on with great suddenness as the result of occlusion of the arteries, of injury to certain parts of the heart, or from strong electrical stimulation. Fibrillation of the auricles also occurs frequently under experimental conditions, but the musculature in this part of the heart seems to be able to return to its normal co-ordinated contractions with much less difficulty. The cause of the sudden * For a description of results and the literature see Porter, " Journal of Physiology," 15, 121, 1S93; also "Journal of Experimental Medicine," 1, 1, 1896. CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. change from co-ortinated-to fibrillar contractions has never been SSftS : thaettred' hl ^ C°nneCti0n h is interest recall also that when any injury is done to either ventricle suf ficient to stop the contractions or to cause fibrillation both ven nc es stop togeth This ^ .g ^ on both ven- boSl^r1^ * aftGr al1 °ne SGt °f fib- "^ CHAPTER XXIX. THE CAUSE AND THE SEQUENCE OF THE HEART BEAT— PROPERTIES OF THE HEART MUSCLE. General Statement. — The cause of the heart beat has naturally constituted one of the fundamental objects of physiological inquiry. The various views that have been proposed in different centuries reflect more or less accurately the advancement of the science. With each new discovery of general significance a new point of view is obtained and the theories of the heart beat, like those of the other great problems of physiology, shift their standpoint from generation to generation. The general modern conception of this problem is referred usually to Haller (1757), who first taught that the activity of the heart is not dependent on its connections with the central nervous system. As we shall see, the heart beat is controlled and influenced constantly by the central nervous system, but never- theless the important point has been established beyond question that the heart continues to beat when all these nervous connections are severed. The central nervous system regulates the activity of the heart, but has nothing to do with the cause of its rhythmical contractions. The heart, in other words, is an automatic organ. When in 1848 Remak discovered that nerve cells are contained in the frog's heart it was natural that the causation of the beat should be attributed to this tissue. Subsequent histological work has demonstrated the existence of numerous nerve cells in the substance of the heart tissue of all vertebrates, and the view that the au- tomaticity of the heart is due in reality to the properties of the contained nerve cells was the prevalent view throughout the middle and latter part of the nineteenth century. In the latter part of the century an opposite view arose, — namely, that the muscular tissue of the heart itself possesses the property of automatic rhythmical contractility. Both these points of view persist to dajr. The theory that refers the automaticity of the heart beat to the contained nerve cells is designated as the neurogenic theory of the heart beat; the one that refers this property to the muscle tissue itself is known as the myogenic theory. Beyond this question lies the still deeper problem of the explanation of the automaticity itself, the cause or causes of the rhythmical excitation, whether occurring primarily in the muscle cells or in the nerve cells. 543 544 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. The dividing line between the ancient and the modern views of the heart beat is fonnd in the work of William Harvey (1628). Before his time physi- cians thought along the lines laid down by the ancient masters, Hippocrates, Aristotle, and Galen, in that they believed that the diastole of the heart is the active part of the beat. They believed that the heart dilated at the mo- ment of the apex beat, the dilatation being due to the implanted heat, the vital spirits, a special pulsatile force, etc. The arteries dilated at the same time for a similar reason. For a period of over two thousand years men's minds were so chained to this belief that they apparently could take no other view. Harvey, liowever, had the boldness and originality to look at the matter differently. He saw and proved that the active movement of the heart is a contraction during systole, which drives blood out of the ventricles into the arteries, and consequently that the pulse of the arteries is not clue to their active dilatation, but to a distension by the blood forced into them. Harvey may be considered also as the founder of the myogenic theory of the heart beat. For although he did not speculate concerning the cause of the beat, he taught that the" systole is an active contraction of the heart's own muscu- lature not dependent upon any external influence. In the same century the first neurogenic hypothesis was formulated. AYillis conceived that the cere- bellum controls the activity of the involuntary organs, including the heart. The animal spirits engendered in the cerebellum were conveyed to the heart by the vagus nerve and catised its beat. Borelli formulated a somewhat different view. According to him the nerve juice, succus spirituosus, elabo- rated in the central nervous system was transmitted to the heart through the cardiac nerves and, distilling slowly into the musculature, set up an ebullition which caused an active expansion'of the fibers. This expansion constituted the systole and drove the blood out of the heart. Both of these views were disproved or rendered improbable largely by the work of Haller, who in 1757 published the second myogenic theory in a form which, somewhat modified, prevails to-day. Haller believed that 'the contraction of the heart is due to the inherent irritability of its musculature, ami that the venous blood as it enters the heart stimulates it to contraction. Haller's views were generally accepted for some years, but some physiologists continued to believe that the heart beat i- controlled directly by the central nervous system. This theory found its most definite expression in the work of Legallois, 1812, who advanced what may be called the second neurogenic hypothesis. From experiments made upon animals he concluded that the principle or force that causes the heart beat is formed in the spinal cord, in all <>!' its parts, and reaches the heart- through the branches of the sympathetic nerve supplying this organ. Legallois's conclusions were soon shown to be erroneous, but the general view advocated by him was entertained by some as late as the middle of the H»th century, in fact until experimental physiology had demonstrated the true functions of the vagus and accelerator nerves with reference to the heart. Toward the middle of the I'.ith century a third form of neurogenic hypothesis arose, which in the beginning seems to have been due to the work or the system of Bichat. According to this author the ganglionic or sympathetic system supplies the of the organic life, meaning thereby the visceral organs which are not under the direel influence of the will. In' IMI Etemak discovered thai the heart possesses intrinsic nerve ganglia, and this fact seems to have induced most physiologists to believe thai these ganglia constitute a motor center for the heart, initiating and CO ordinating its heat. for a period of forty years this form of the neurogenic hypothesis enjoyed almost universal acceptance. In issi 83Gaskell published experiments upon the heart of the frog and tortoise in which he gave strong reasons for believing thai the beat ismyogenicin origin, and thai the intrinsic ganglia are simply .-i pari of the inhibitory ap- paratus of the heart. Since thai time many physiologists have adopted the myogenic view, and the currenl arguments tending to support this rather than the neurogenic hypothesis are presented in the text. The most significant addition to our knowledge <>i the cause of the- heart heat made during the L-, t quarter of a century is the discovery thai the inorganic salts of the blood and lymph play a special and essential r61e. The facts hearing upon this interesting cuscoa ery are sufficiently described in I he text. PROPERTIES OF THE HEART MUSCLE. 545 The Neurogenic Theory of the Heart Beat. — The literature upon this topic is very large.* The neurogenic theory has suffered some changes in its details since first proposed by Volkmann, particularly in the specific functions assigned to the different ganglia that exist in the heart. In general, however, the theory assumes that the excitation to each beat arises within the nerve cells, and since the cardiac cycle begins with a contraction at what may be called the venous end of the heart, — that is, at the junction of the veins with the auricles, — it is assumed that the excitation or inner stimulus arises in the nerve cells situated in this region. These cells constitute, therefore, what may be called the automatic motor center of the heart. The stimuli generated within it are transmitted through its axons first to the musculature of the venous end of the heart. The subsequent orderly march of this contraction, to auricles and then to ventricles, is also upon this theory usually attributed to the intrinsic nerve cells and fibers. Through a definite mechanism the impulses generated in the motor center are trans- mitted to subordinate nerve centers through which the auricles are excited, and then to other nerve cells lying in or near the auriculo- ventricular groove through which the ventricles are excited. In this form the theory assumes for the heart an intrinsic central nervous system, as it were, with a principal motor center in which the property of automaticity is chiefly developed and subordinate centers whose activity usually depends upon the principal center, but which may show automatic properties of a lower order if the connections between them and the main center are interrupted. This intrinsic nervous system is responsible not only for the spon- taneous origination and normal sequence of the beat, but also for its co-ordination. The many muscular fibers of the ventricle contract normally in a definite manner and sequence, so that their effect is summated. Under abnormal conditions the fibers may contract irregularly, giving the so-called fibrillar contractions of the heart, which are inco-ordinatecl. It may be said that this con- ception of the connections of the intrinsic nervous system rests mainly upon deductions from physiological experiments. The histological details regarding the connections of the nerve cells in the heart are not yet sufficiently known, but it can not be said at present that they give any positive support to such a view. In regard to the neurogenic theory the following general statements may be made : 1. Most of the very numerous facts known regarding the heart * For recent general presentations from different standpoints see Gaskell, article on "The Contraction of Cardiac Muscle," in Schafer's "Text-book of Physiology," vol. ii. 1900; Langendorff, " Herzmuskel und intrakardiale In- nervation" in "Ergebnisse der Physiologie," vol. i, part n, 1902; and Cyon, "L'innervation du cceur," Richet's " Dictionnaire du Phvsiologie," vol. iv, 1900. 35 546 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. beat and its variations under experimental conditions may be explained in terms of the theory, or at least do not contradict it. The same statement, however, may be made regarding the myogenic theory. Both theories may be applied successfully from a logical standpoint to the explanation of known facts. 2. No single fact is known which can be cited as positive proof that the nerves participate in the production of the normal beat of the vertebrate heart. The experiment by Kronecker and Schmey is sometimes given this significance. These observers have shown that, when a needle is thrust into a certain spot in the dog's ventricle, the regularly contracting heart falls suddenly into fibrillar contractions so far as the ventricles are concerned. The ex- periment is certainly a striking and interesting one. The needle may be thrust many times into certain portions of the muscu- lar mass without affecting the powerful co-ordinated contractions, but in the region specified by Kronecker a single puncture, if it reaches the right spot, causes the ventricle to fall into ir- regular fibrillar twitches from which it does not recover. The spot as described by Kronecker is along the line of the septum at the lower border of its upper third. The experiment frequently fails; and it would seem that there must be a definite and quite circum- scribed structure whose lesion produces the effect described. We have no evidence as yet what this structure is, and are therefore in no condition to make positive inferences with regard to the bearing of the experiment upon the origin of the heart beat. Carlson * has described experiments upon the heart of the horseshoe crab (Limulus) which seem to show conclusively that in this animal the rhythmical contractions are dependent upon the intrinsic nerve cells. These latter are placed superficially, forming a cord that runs the length of the tubular heart. When this cord is removed the heart ceases to beat. There are reasons, however, which at present make it doubtful whether we can apply the results of this experiment to the vertebrate heart. The crustacean heart differs from the vertebrate heart in its fundamental properties; unlike the latter, it lias no refractory period (see p. 552), can be tetanized, and gives submaximal contract ions. f It is a tissue, therefore, that resembles in its properties ordinary skeletal muscle in the verte- brate, and, like this muscle, it, seems to be lacking in automaticity. Carlson's experiments liive, however, another instance of automatic rhythmicity in nerve tissue, and to that extent support the neuro- genic theory. The Myogenic Theory of the Heart Beat. — The myogenic ♦ Carlson, "American Journal <>f Physiology," 12, 67, and 471, L905, | Hunt, Bookman, and Tierney, "Centralblutt f. Physiologic," 11, 275, 1897. PROPERTIES OF THE HEART MUSCLE. 547 theory has been developed chiefly by Gaskell and by Engelmann. It assumes that the heart muscle itself possesses the property of automatic rhythmicity and that this property is most highly de- veloped at the venous end. This portion of the heart, therefore, contracts first and the wave of contraction spreads directly to the musculature of the auricle and thence to that of the ventricle. The quickly beating venous end sets the pace, as it were, for the entire heart. The nerve cells and nerve fibers that are present in the heart are upon this theory supposed to be connected with the extrinsic nerves through which the rate and force of the heart beat are regu- lated, but they are not concerned in the production of the beat. Many experimental facts have been accumulated which give probability to this view, and it has been adopted by many, perhaps most, of the recent workers in this field. Some of the facts that favor this theory are as follows: 1. The anatomical arrangement of the musculature of the heart is not opposed to such a theory. It was formerly stated quite positively that there is no muscular connection between the auricles and ventricles in the mammalian heart, but a number of observers have now demonstrated the existence of a muscular bridge, the auriculo-ventricular bundle, between the two chambers (see p. 520) . 2. The fact that a contraction started at one part of the heart may travel to other portions through the intervening musculature may be said to be demonstrated. Thus, Engelmann has shown that if the ventricle in the frog's heart is cut in a zigzag fashion, so that strips are obtained which are connected only by narrow bridges, a stimulation applied at one end starts a wave of con- traction which propagates itself over all of the pieces. This and similar experiments scarcely permit of explanation on the supposi- tion that conduction from piece to piece is effected by a definite nervous mechanism. So too it has been shown that under certain conditions the normal auriculo-ventricular rhythm can be changed at will to a ventriculo-auricular rhythm. If, for instance, a ligature be tied around the frog's heart between the sinus venosus and the auricle (first ligature of Stannius) the auricle and ventricle cease to beat. In this quiescent condition a slight mechanical stimulus to the ventricle causes it to beat and its contraction is immediately followed by that of the auricle. A similar reversed rhythm may be obtained from the mammalian heart under suitable conditions. Such an experiment makes it most probable that the contraction is propagated from one chamber to the other directly through the muscular connections. It is not possible at present to conceive that a definite mechanism of neurons should work thus in either direction. 3. There is much probable proof that the heart muscle tissue possesses the property of automatic rhythmical contractions. Ex- 5-AS CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. periments, initiated by Gaskell and since extended by numerous observers, show that in the cold-blooded animals strips of heart muscle taken from various parts of the heart will under proper conditions develop rhythmical contractions. It is very improbable that each of these strips, no matter how made, contains its own resident nerve evils or nerve tissue which act as a motor center. These results seem to demonstrate an inherent property of rhythm- icity in cardiac muscle, whether or not this rhythmicity is directly responsible for the normal beat. 4. It has been shown that in the embryo chick the heart pul- sates normally before the nerve cells have grown into it, and it is stated that in the hearts of a number of invertebrates no nerve cells can be found. Much weight can not be given, however, to negative evidence of this kind, since, in the first place, better technical methods may demonstrate the existence of such cells, and even if absent from the heart itself it is conceivable that they may be present in the surrounding tissue and send their fibers to the heart. It is evident from this brief and imperfect presenta- tion that it is not possible to claim that either the neurogenic or the myogenic theory is demonstrated, but most physiologists per- haps at present believe that the latter view is more in accaii with the facts.* Automaticity of the Heart. — As was said above, the ques- tion of the cause or causes of the automatic rhythmical con- tractions must be sought for whether the phenomenon turns out to be a property of the muscular tissue or of the nervous tissue of the heart. When we say that a given tissue is automatic we mean that the stimuli which excite it to activity arise within the tissue itself, and are not brought to it through extrinsic nerves. In the heart, therefore, we assume that a stimulus is continually being produced, and we speak of it as the inner stimulus. Experiment and speculation have been directed toward unraveling the nature of this inner stimulus. Most of the physiologists who have expressed an opinion upon the subject have sought an explanation in the composil ion of the I flood or lymph bathing the heart tissue, or in the products of metabolism of the tissue itself. Regarding this latter view there i.s nothing of the nature of direel experimental evidence in its favor. No product of the metabolism of the heart tissue capable of exerting this stimulating effect has been isolated. In I In the former view, that the inner stimulus is connected with ;i definite composition of the blood or lymph, there has been con iderable experimental work which is of fundamental signifi- cance. While the older physiologists paid .-it lent ion mainly to (,1k; compromise view, partly myogenic and partly neurogenic, sec J redericq, "ArchiveB internationalea de Physiologie, " 1906, iv., 57. PROPERTIES OF THE HEART MUSCLE. 549 organic substances in the blood, it has been shown in recent years that the inorganic salts are the elements whose influence upon the heart beat is most striking. These salts are in solution in the liquid of the tissue, and are therefore probably more or less com- pletely dissociated. Attention has been directed mainly to the influence of the cations, of which three are especially important, — namely, the sodium, the calcium, and the potassium. The Action of the Calcium, Potassium, and Sodium Ions in the Blood and Lymph. — It has long been known that the heart of a frog or terrapin may be kept beating normally for hours after removal from the body, provided it is supplied with an artificial circulation of blood or lymph, so arranged that this liquid enters the heart through the veins from a reservoir of some sort and is pumped out through the arteries leading from the ventricle. It was first shown by Merunowicz, working under Ludwig's direction, that an aqueous extract of the ash of the blood possesses a similar action. Ringer afterwards proved that the frog's heart can be kept beating for long periods upon a mixture of sodium chlorid, potassium chlorid, and calcium phosphate or chlorid, and he laid especial stress upon the importance of the calcium. This work was after- wards confirmed and extended by Howell, Loeb, and others, who attempted to analyze the part played by the several ions.* If a frog's or terrapin's heart is fed with a solution of physiological saline (NaCl, 0.7 per cent.) it beats well for a while, but the beats soon weaken and gradually fade out. If in this condition the heart is fed with a proper mixture of sodium, potassium, and calcium chlorids it beats vigorously and well for very many hours. A solution containing these three salts in proper propor- tions is known usually as Ringer's mixture. The exact com- position has been varied by different workers, but for the heart of the frog or terrapin the following composition is most effective: NaCl = 0.7 per cent. KC1 = 0.03 " " CaCl = 0.025 " " The addition of a trace of alkali, HNaC03, 0.003 per cent., often increases the effectiveness of the solution, but it can not be considered an essential constituent in the same sense as sodium, potassium, and calcium. It has been shown, moreover, that even the mammalian heart can be kept beating for long periods when fed with a Ringer solution if provision is made for a larger supply * For literature and discussion see Howell, "American Journal of Phys- iology," 2, 47, 1898, and 6, 181, 1901, and "Journal of the American Medical Association," 1906. 550 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. of oxygen than can be obtained by simple exposure to the air. For the irrigation of the isolated mammalian heart different forms of Ringer's solution have been employed, but the mixture most frequently used is that recommended by Locke, consisting of NaCl, 0.9 per cent.; CaCl2, 0.024 per cent.; KC1, 0.042 per cent.; XaHCO.,, 0.01 to 0.03 per cent.; and dextrose, 0.1 per cent. The solution is fed to the heart under an atmosphere of oxygen, and with this solution Locke and others have kept the mammalian heart beating for many hours. The dextrose, while not essential to the action of the irrigating liquid, is said to increase its effi- ciency, and Locke* has shown that the sugar is apparently utilized by the heart, since a considerable amount disappears from the solution when the heart is beating strongly. The general fact that comes out of these experiments is that the heart can beat for very long periods upon what has been called an inorganic diet. Moreover, the salts that are used cannot be chosen at random; it is necessary to have salts of the three metals named, and substitution is possible only to a very limited ex- tent. Thus, strontium salts may replace those of calcium more or less perfectly. It is evident that these salts play some very important part in the production of the rhythmical beat of the heart; and analysis has shown that the sodium, calcium, and potassium has each its special role. We may say that the presence of these salts in normal proportions is an absolute necessity for heart activity. A striking experiment which shows the importance of the calcium is to irrigate a terrapin's heart with blood from which the calcium has been removed by precipitation with sodium oxalate. In spite of the fact that all other constituents of the blood are present the heart ceases to beat, and normal contractions can be started again promptly by adding calcium chlorid in right amounts to the oxalated blood. Regarding the specific part taken by each of the cations in the production of the alternate contractions and relaxations, much diversity of opinion exists, owing to our ignorance of the chemical changes going on in the heart during systole and diastole and to the difficulty of controlling experimental conditions. Thus, while it is an easy matter to control accurately the corn- posit ion of the liquids supplied to the heart, a variable and uncon- trollable factor is introduced by the fact that within the tissue elements themselves there is a store of combined calcium, potassium, and sodium which may serve to supply these elements to a greater or less extent to Ihe tissue liquids. The controversial details upon this question can not be presented in an elementary book, but the following brief statements may be made regarding one view of the specific effects of the separate * Locke :ui'l Ro enheiin, "Journal <>f Physiology, " 36, 2().r>, |<)()7. PROPERTIES OF THE HEART MUSCLE. 551 cations: (1) The sodium salts in the blood and lymph take the chief part in the maintenance of normal osmotic pressure. The sodium chlorid exists in blood-plasma to the extent of 0.5 to 0.6 per cent., and the normal osmotic pressure of the blood is mainly- dependent upon it. A solution of sodium chlorid of 0.7 to 0.9 per cent, forms what is known as physiological saline, and although not adequate to maintain the normal composition and properties of the tissues it fulfills this purpose more perfectly than the solution of any other single substance. The sodium ions have in addition a specific influence upon the state of the heart tissue. Contractility and irritability disappear when they are absent ; when present alone, in physiological concentration, in the medium bathing the heart mus- cles they produce relaxation of the muscle tissue. (2) The calcium ions are present in relatively very small quantities in the blood, but they also are absolutely necessary to contractility and irritability. When present in quantities above normal or when in a propor- tional excess over the sodium or potassium ions they cause a con- dition of tonic contraction that has been designated as calcium rigor. (3) The potassium ions are present also in very small quan- tities, and, unlike the calcium and sodium ions, their presence in the circulating liquid does not seem to be absolutely necessary to rhythmical activity. Under proper conditions a terrapin's heart beats well for a time upon a solution containing only sodium and calcium salts. The potassium seems to promote relaxation of the muscle and in physiological doses it exercises through this effect a regulating influence upon the rate of beat. When the proportion of potassium ions is increased the heart rate is proportionally slowed, and finally the contractions cease altogether, the heart coming to rest in a state of extreme relaxation, known sometimes as potassium inhibition. (4) It appears from these statements that there is a well-marked antagonism between the effects of the calcium, on the one hand, and the potassium and sodium, on the other. The calcium promotes a state of contraction, the sodium and the potassium a state of relaxation. It is conceivable, there- fore, that the alternate states of contraction and relaxation which characterize the rhythmical action of heart muscle are connected in some way with an interaction of an alternating kind between these ions and the living contractile substance of the heart. It is impossible to say positively whether or not the inorganic salts are directly connected with the cause of the beat, — that is, with the origination of the inner stimulus. According to one point of view, they are necessary only to the irritability and contractility of the heart tissue. The inner stimulus is produced otherwise by some unknown reaction, but it is not able to cause a contraction of the heart muscle in the absence of the proper inorganic salts. According to another view, the reaction of these ions with the 552 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. living substance constitutes or leads to the development of the inner stimulus. Physiological Properties of Cardiac Muscle. — Cardiac muscle exhibits certain properties which distinguish it sharply from skeletal muscular tissue and which have a direct bearing upon the rhyth- micity of the contractions and the sequence shown by the different chambers. The most characteristic of these properties are the following: 1. The contractions of heart muscle are always maximal. In skeletal muscle and in plain muscle the extent of contraction is related to the strength of the stimulus, and we recognize the exis- tence of a series of submaximal contractions of varying heights. This is not true of heart muscle. As was first shown by Bow- ditch, a piece of ventricular muscle when stimulated responds, if it responds at all, with a maximal contraction. The apex of a frog's heart does not beat spontaneously, but contracts upon electrical stimulation. If such an apex is connected with a lever to register its contractions, and the electrical stimulus applied to it is gradually increased, the first contraction to appear is maxi- mal, and it is not further increased by augmenting the stimulus. This property is sometimes described by saying (Ranvier) that the contraction of the heart muscle is all or none. This fact must not, however, be interpreted to mean that the force of contraction of heart muscle is invariable under all conditions. Such is not the case. The heart muscle under favorable nutritive conditions may give a much larger and more forcible contraction than is possible under conditions of poor nutrition; but the point is, that, whatever may be the condition of the muscle at any given moment, its contraction in response to artificial stimulation is maximal for that condition, — that is, does not vary with the strength of the stimulus. As was said above, this property is not exhibited by the crustacean (lobster) heart, but has been shown to be true for the mammalian heart muscle.* 2. The refractory period of the beat. It was shown by Marey f that the heart muscle is irritable to artificial (electrical) stimuli only during the period of diastole. During the period of systole an elec- trical stimulus lias no effect; during the period of diastole such a stimulus calls forth an extra contraction and the latent period preceding the extra contraction is shorter the later the stimulus is applied in the diastolic phase. This relationship is well shown by Marey's curves reproduced in fig. 228. The period of inexcitability U designated as the refractory period of (he hearl heat. Marey * For experiments on mammalian heart and literature, woo Wood worth, " American Journal of Physiology," 8, 213, 1903. t Marey, "Travaux 'lu laboratoire," 1876, \>. 73. PROPERTIES OF THE HEART MUSCLE. 553 defined this refractory period as falling within the first part of the systole, and stated that its duration varies with the actual strength Pig- 228. — To show the effect of a short electrical stimulus applied at different times in the heart beat. — (Marey.) The record is taken from the frog's heart. In 1, 2, and 3 the stimulus (e) falls into the heart during systole (refractory period) and has no effect. In 4. 5. 6, 7, and S the stimulus falls into the heart toward the end of systole or during diastole, and is followed by an extra systole and corresponding compensatory pause. It will be noted that the latent period (shaded area) between the stimulus and the extra systole is shorter the longer the diastole has proceded before the stimulus is applied. 55-1 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. of the stimulus. Later experiments by other investigators make it probable that the refractory period lasts during practically the entire systole.* According to this point of view, therefore, the heart muscle during its period of actual contraction is entirely unirritable, and in this respect it offers a striking difference to skeletal and plain muscle. The existence of this refractory period explains why the heart muscle cannot be thrown into complete tetanic contractions by rapidly repeated stimuli. Since each contraction is accompanied by a condition of loss of irritability, it is obvious that those stimuli that fall into the heart during this period must prove ineffective. The refractory period and the gradual increase in irritability during the diastole may throw some light also on the rhythmical character of the beat. The occurrence of the refractory period and the subsequent gradual return of irritability are connected no doubt with the metabolic changes taking place in the heart muscle. It is in the character of this metabolism that we must seek for the final explanation of these two phenomena and the cause of the rhythmicity of the contractions. As was stated above, it has been shown that the crustacean (lobster) heart muscle does not obey the all-or-none law, shows no refractory period, and is capable of giving tetanic contractions when rapidly stimulated. In all these respects it differs from the typical heart muscle of the vertebrate, but the difference is perhaps sufficiently explained by the discovery (p. 546) that the crustacean heart, in one form at least, is not an automatically rhythmical tissue. Its rhythmical contractions, like those of the diaphragmatic muscle in the higher vertebrates, depend upon rhythmical impulses received from nerve centers. The Compensatory Pause. — It has been observed that when an extra systole is produced by stimulating a ventricle it is followed by a pause longer than usual; the pause, in fact, is of such a length as to compensate exactly for the extra beat; so that the total rate of beat remains the same. The pro- longed pause under these conditions is therefore frequently designated as the compensatory i«nix(>. It has been shown,t however, that the exact compen- sation in this case is not referable to a property of heart muscle, but is due to the dependence of the ventricular upon the auricular beat. When the auricle or ventricle is isolated and stimulated the phenomenon of exact compensation IS n"i observed. In an entire heart, on the contrary, the beat originates at th<- venous em I of the auricle and is propagated to the ventricle. If the latter chamb< r i- stimulated so as to give an extra beat out of sequence it will remain in diastole until the next auricular beat stimulates it, and will thus pick up the regular sequence of the heart beat. The Normal Sequence of the Heart Beat. -The normal rhythm of the hearl beal is firsl a contraction of the auricles, then one of the ventricles. Many efforts have Ween made to determine the precise spot in which the contraction of the heart * Bee paper by Woodworth, he. cit. Also Schultz, "American Journal of Phyriology,'5 16, 483, I906,and22, L33, L908. f Cushny and Matthews, "Journal of Physiology," 21, 227, L897. PROPERTIES OF THE HEART MUSCLE. 555 normally starts. Formerly it was supposed that the contraction began in the great veins just before they pass into the auricle, and it was implied that this initiation of the beat might occur in the pulmonary veins as well as in the vense cavse. More recent experiments* which have been made largely upon the isolated heart while perfused with a Ringer-Locke solution have shown pretty conclusively that the most rhythmic part of the heart and the part from which the beat, in all probability, normally starts is an area of the wall of the right auricle lying between the openings of the vense cavse. When this portion of the heart is warmed or cooled the rate of beat of the whole heart is cor- respondingly increased or decreased, while, on the contrary, warming or cooling of the ventricles themselves, the auricular appendages, the left auricle, etc., has no effect upon the heart- rate. From the point of confluence of the vense cavse the wave of contraction spreads over the auricles and thence through the auriculoventricular bundle to the ventricles. This sequence from venous to arterial end is beautifully shown in the frog's heart, in which the contraction begins in the sinus venosus, spreads to the auricles, thence to the ventricle, and finally to the bulbus arteriosus. Under normal conditions this sequence is never reversed, and an explanation of the natural order forms obviously an important part of any complete theory of the heart beat. Those who hold to the neurogenic theory naturally explain the sequence of the beat by reference to the intrinsic nervous apparatus. If the motor ganglia lie toward the venous end of the"heart one can imagine that their discharges may affect the different chambers in sequence, the pause between auricular and ventricular contraction being due, let us say, to the fact that the motor impulses to the ventricle have to act through subordinate nerve cells in the auriculo-ventricular region, and the time necessary for this action brings the ventricular contraction a certain interval later than that of the auricle. There is no immediate proof or disproof of such a view. The numerous exper- iments made upon the rapidity of conduction of the wave of contraction over the heart are not conclusive either for or against the view. The fact, however, that in the quiescent but still irritable heart the rhythm may be reversed by artificially stimulating the ventricle first seems to the author to speak strongly against the dependence of the sequence upon any definite arrangement of neuron complexes. On the myogenic theory the sequence of the heart beat is accounted for readily by relatively simple assumptions. Gaskell and Engelmann have each laid emphasis upon the facts in ♦Consult especially Adam, "Archiv f. d. ges. Physiol.," Ill, 607, 1906, and Erlanger and Blackman, "American Journal of Physiology, " 19, 125, 1907. 556 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. this connection, and the application of the myogenic theory to the explanation of the normal sequence of contractions forms one of its most attractive features. Gaskell assumes* that the rhythmical power of the muscle at the venous end is greater than that at the ventricular end, — that is, if pieces from the two ends are examined separately it will be found that the spontaneous rhythm of the tissue from the venous end is more rapid. This portion of the heart, therefore, beating more rapidly, sets the rhythm for the whole organ, since a contraction started at the venous end will propagate itself from chamber to chamber. The pause between the contractions of the successive chambers — between auricle and ventricle, for instance — is due, in the heart of the tortoise, to the fact that the muscular tissue at the junction of auricle and ventricle has a relatively low rate of conduction. At this point, indeed, the muscular fibers form a ring around the orifice, preserving, therefore, the arrangement found in the embryo at the time that the heart has the form of a tube. Gaskell has given reasons for believing that the conduction of the wave of contraction is slower through this ring. In the mammalian heart the direct conduc- tion of the wave of contraction from auricle to ventricle through intervening muscular tissue is made quite possible, since the discovery has been made that the auricles and ventricles are connected by a muscular bundle (p. 520). If with Gaskell we assume that the conduction through this bundle is slower than it is over the surface of the auricle or ventricle, then the pause between auricular and ventricular systole is sufficiently explained. That each chamber of the heart has a rhythm of its own and that the rhythm of the venous end is the more rapid and constitutes the rhythm of the intact heart has been shown in various ways upon the hearts of different animals. Thus, Tigerstedt has devised an instrument, the atriotome,* by means of which the connections between auricle and ventricle may be crushed without hemorrhage. Under such conditions the ventricle continues to beat, but with a much slower rhythm and with a rhythm entirely independent of that of the auricles. The same re- sult has been obtained recently in a very striking way by Erlanger. This observer arranged a clamp by means of which he could com- press the -mall bundle of fibers connecting auricle and ventricle. When the compression is made the ventricle, after an interval, exhibits a slower rhythm and one entirely independent of that of the auricles. When the compression is removed the ventricle falls in again with the auricular rhythm. By vaiia! ions in the pressure ♦ Gaskell, " Journal of Physiology," •, 61, L883; also vol. ii., p. 180, of Schafi l ■ I I«>"1< of Physiology/' L900 Lehrbucb <1f an experiment in which the vagi were compressed in the neck, with a resulting Stoppage of the heart and loss of consciousness. Fig. 231. — To show the inhibit ion of the terrapin's heart due to stimula- tion of the vagus nerve. The upper tracing '/) records t he contractions of the left auricle; the lower (//) the con- t ructions of the ventricle. The vagus was stimulated three times, each chamber coming to u complete stop. On removing the stimulus it will be noted thai the auricular contraction increase gradually to their normal, while the ventricular contractions ■tail off at full strength. THE CARDIAC NERVES. 563 especially in the terrapin, the inhibitory fibers may be found exclusively or mainly in the right vagus. Analysis of the Action of the Inhibitory Fibers.— The prom- inent effect of the action of the inhibitory fibers is the slowing Fig. 232. — To show the inhibition of the heart from stimulation of the vagus in the dog. Record B is the blood-pressure tracing. The vagus was stimulated twice. The marks x, x, indicate the beginning and end of the stimulus. The first stimulation was weak ; it will be noted that the heart escaped and began beating before the stimulus was withdrawn. The second stimulus was stronger; the inhibition lasted some time after re- moval of the stimulus. The upper curve (A") is a plethysmography (oncometer) tracing of the volume of the kidnev. It will be noted that when the heart stops and blood-pressure falls the kidney, like the other organs, diminishes in volume. (Dawson.) of the rate of the heart beat. Numerous observers have called attention to the fact that the vagus fibers may also cause a weaken- ing in the force of the beat as well as a slowing in the rate, or, indeed, the two effects may be obtained separately. This fact has been shown especially for the auricles.* In the heart of the terrapin one may, by using weak stimuli, obtain only a weakening of the *See Bayliss and Starling, "Journal of Physiology," 13, 410, 1892. 564 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. auricular beats without any interference with the rate (Fig. 233), while by increasing the stimulus the slowing in rate becomes evident combined with a diminution in force or extent. Although the force of the beat ma}' be influenced without altering the rate, the reverse does not hold. Usually, for the auricle, at least, any stimulus that slows the beat also weakens the individual beat. Whether the vagus fibers exercise a similar double influence directly upon the ventricle is not so clear. Some observers find that when the ventricle is inhibited the beats, although slower, are stronger, while others obtain an opposite result. It seems probable, as stated by Johansson and Tigerstedt, that the result obtained depends largely on the strength of stimulus used. These observers found* that with relative^ weak stimuli the contractions of the ventricle, though slower, are stronger, while with stronger stimuli the contractions are diminished in strength as well as rate. The question is complicated by the difficulty of separating the direct effect of the vagus on the ventricle from the indirect effect brought about by the changes in the auricular beat. The inhibitory in- fluence makes itself felt also upon the conductivity of the heart. This fact has been noted by several ob- servers. A striking example is seen in the case of partial heart block. When as the result of some injury or pressure in the auriculo-ventricu- lar region or from some other less evident cause there is a partial block, so that the ventricle con- fcracts once to two or three beats of the auricle, vagus stimulation may be followed a1 once, as :m after-effect, by a return to the normal beat, a re-establishment of a one-to-one rhythm. Under other circumstances the contrary effect of vagus stimulation I,-,- been described. From the results cited it seems evident that the vagus nerve may affect the rate and the force of the con- tractions, and also the conductivity or the propagation of the wave of contraction. These separate influences have been referred by some authors to the existence of different kinds of nerve fibers, each exerting its own influence, but it seems preferable to assume, e i iger fcedt , ' Lehrbuch der Physiologie des Kreislaufes," 1893, p. 247. Fig. 2:3.*.— To show the effect of vagus stimulation on the force only of the auricular beat in the terrapin's heart: A, Record of the auricular beats; V, record of the ventricular beats. The vagus was stimulated be- tween x and x. It will be noted that the ventricular beats are not affected, and that the auricular beats diminish in extent without any change in rate. THE CARDIAC NERVES. 565 on the contrary, that only one kind of fiber is present, and that its influence on the metabolic changes in the heart muscle expresses itself differently upon the several different properties of the tissue according to the extent of its action. Engelmann has made the most complete attempt to analyze the influence exerted by the cardiac nerves (inhibitory and accelerator). He designates these influences under four different heads with the further supposition that they are mediated by different fibers : (1) The chronotropic influence, affecting the rate of contraction, positive chronotropic actions causing an acceleration and negative chronotropic actions a slowing of the rate. (2) The bathmo- tropic influence, affecting the irritability of the muscular tissue ; this also may be positive or negative. (3) The dromotropic influence, positive or negative, affecting the conductivity of the tissue. (4) The inotropic influence, posi- tive or negative, affecting the force or energy of the contractions.* Does the Vagus Affect Both Auricle and Ventricle? — The inhibitory action of the vagus is most marked upon the venous end of the heart, and the question has arisen as to whether it affects the ventricle directly or not. Gaskell gave evidence to indicate that in the terrapin the auricle only is inhibited, the ventricle stop- ping because it fails to receive its normal impulse from the auricle. When this heart is inhibited the contractions of the auricle after cessation of inhibition gradually increase in amplitude until the normal size is reached; in the ventricle, on the contrary, the first contraction after inhibition is of normal size or greater than normal (see Fig. 231). When a block is produced in the mammalian heart between auricle and ventricle — by clamping the connecting muscular bundle, for instance — stimulation of the vagus stops the auricle only (Erlanger), and this result would seem to indicate that the vagus affects only the auricle, unless it is as- sumed that the clamp has interrupted the inhibitory paths to the ventricle. On the other hand, in favor of the view that the vagus fibers reach the ventricle and influence its beats directly, we have the fact, emphasized by Tigerstedt, namely, that when the connection between auricle and ventricle is severed suddenly the ventricle frequently continues to beat at its own rhythm without any obvious pause. It would seem from this fact that when the whole heart is inhibited by stimulation of the vagus the ventricle does not stop simply because the auricle fails to send on its usual contraction wave, since, if that were so, cutting off the auricle or clamping the connection between it and the ventricle should also bring on a ventricular pause, as happens in the case of the terrapin's heart. It seems, however, to be the general belief of those who have experi- mented with the subject that the action of the vagus is exerted mainly upon the auricles. * Engelmann, 'Archiv f. Physiologie, " 1900, p. 313, and 1902, suppl. volume, p. 1. 566 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. Escape from Inhibition.— Strong stimulation of the vagus may stop the entire heart, but the length of time during which the heart may be maintained in this condition varies in different species and indeed to some extent in different individuals.* In some ani- mals— eats, for example — the strongest stimulation of the nerve serves frequently only to slow the heart instead of causing complete standstill. In dogs the heart is stopped by relatively weak stimu- lation, although if the stimulation is maintained the heart, as a rule, escapes from the inhibition. In some dogs the heart may be held inhibited long enough to cause the death of the animal unless artificial respiration is maintained, but usually the heart beat soon breaks through the complete inhibition. The "inner stimulus" in such cases increases in strength sufficiently to overcome the opposing inhibitory influence, and this circumstance may be regarded as an argument against those views that trace the origin of the "inner stimulus" to some of the products formed during the ca- tabolism of contraction. Moderate stimulation of the vagus, suffi- cient simply to slow the rate of beat, can be maintained without dimi- nution in effect for very long periods; indeed, as is explained in the next paragraph, the heart beat is kept partially inhibited more or less continuously through life by a constant activity of the vagus. In the cold-blooded animals, especially the terrapin, the heart may be kept completely inhibited for hours by stimu- lation of the vagus. Mills reports that he has kept the heart of the terrapin in this condition for more than four hours. f Mos1 observers state that complete inhibition can be maintained for a longer time when the stimulus is applied alternately to the two vagi, but it is possible that this result is due to the fact that continuous stimulation applied to a nerve usually results in some local loss of irritability. Reflex Inhibition of the Heart Beat — Cardio-inhibitory Center. — The inhibitory fibers may be stimulated reflexly by action upon various sensory nerves or surfaces. One of the first experi- mental proofs of this fact was furnished by Goltz's often-quoted " Eflopfversuch."J In this experiment, made upon frogs, the ob- server obtained standstill of the heart by light, rapid taps on the abdomen, and the effect upon the heart failed to appear when the . agj were cut. In the mammals every laboratory worker has had numerous opportunities to observe that stimulation of the central -tump- of sensory nerves may cause a reflex slowing of the heart beat. The effect is usually very marked when the central stump of one vagUS is stimulated, the other vagus being intact. The vague carries afferenl fibers from the thoracic and abdominal i Hough, 'Journal of Physiology," 18, 161, L895. t" Journal of Phyi iology," <>, 246. jGoltz, " Virchow'a Archiv f. pathol. Anatomic, ct,c,"2(i, ll, 1863. THE CARDIAC NERVES. 567 viscera, and most observers state that the heart may be reflexly inhibited most readily by stimulation of the sensory surfaces of the abdominal viscera, by a blow upon the viscera, for example, or by sudden distension of the stomach. In man similar results are noticed very frequently. Acute dyspepsia, inflammation of the peritoneum, painful stimulation of sensory surfaces, — the testes, for instance, or the middle ear, — may cause a marked slowing of the heart, — a condition designated as bradycardia. What takes place in all such cases is that the afferent impulses carried into the central nervous system reflexly stimulate the nerve cells in the medulla which give origin to the inhibitory fibers. These cells form a part of the great motor nucleus (N. ambiguus) from which arise the motor fibers of the vagus and the glossopharyngeus. The particular group of cells from which the inhibitory fibers to the heart originate has not been delimited anatomically. Efforts have been made to locate them by vivisection experiments, but this method has shown no more perhaps than that they are found in the region of origin of the vagus nerve. Physiologically, however, this group of cells forms a center which is of the greatest importance in controlling the activity of the heart. It is designated, therefore, as the cardio-inhibitory center. We may define the cardio-inhibitory center as a bilateral group of cells lying in the medulla at the level of the nucleus of the vagus and giving rise to the inhibitory fibers of the heart. The two sides are probably connected by commis- sural cells or else each nucleus sends fibers to the vagus of each side. Through this center all reflexes that affect the heart by way of the inhibitory fibers must take place. These reflexes may be occa- sioned by incoming sensory impulses through the spinal or cranial nerves, or by impulses coming down from the higher portions of the brain. The center may also be stimulated directly, either by pressure upon the medulla, which may give rise to slow heart beats or, as they are sometimes called, vagal beats, or by changes in the composition of the blood. With regard to the reflex stimulation of this center it is important to bear in mind the general physiological rule that afferent impulses may either excite or inhibit the activity of nerve centers. In the former case the heart rate would be slowed, in the latter case it would be quickened if the center were previously in a state of activity. The Tonic Activity of the Cardio-inhibitory Center. — The cells of the cardio-inhibitory center are in constant activity to a greater or less extent. As a consequence, the heart beat is kept con- tinually at a slower rate than it would normally assume if the inhibitory apparatus did not exist. This tonic activity of the vagus is beautifully exhibited by simple section of the two vagi, or by inter- 568 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. rupting, in some other way — cooling, for example — the connection between the center and the heart. When the two vagi are cut the heart rate increases greatly and the blood-pressure rises on account of the greater output of blood in a unit of time (Fig. 234',. Section of one vagus gives usually a partial effect. — that is, the heart rate is increased somewhat. — but it is still further increased by section of the second vagus. The exact result obtained when the nerves are Fitf. 2'M. — To show the '-fleet of sortion of the two vagi in the dog upon the rate of hear) beat and 1 1 if* blood-pressure: 1 marks the section Ot the vagus on the right side; 2, section of the second VagUS. The numerals on thevertieal mark the blood-pressures; the numerals on the blood-pressure record give the rate of heart beats. (Dawson.) severed separately varies undoubtedly with the conditions, — for instance, with the intensity of tlie tonic activity of the center. Throughout life, speaking in general terms, the cardio-inhibitory center keeps the "brakes" on the heart, rate, and the extent of its action varies under different conditions. When its tonic action is increased the rate: becomes Blower; when it is decreased the rate become- faster. In all probability, this tonic action of the center, like that of the motor centers generally, is in reality a reflex tonus. THE CARDIAC NERVES. 569 That is, it is not due to automatic processes generated within the nerve cells by their own metabolism or by changes in their liquid environment, but to stimulations received through sensory nerves. The continuous though varying inflow of impulses into the central nervous system through different nerve paths keeps the center in that state of permanent gentle activity which we designate as "tone." It is possible, of course, that certain afferent paths may be in specially close functional relationship to the center, and the fact that at each heart beat its own sensory fibers are stimulated (p. 668, Fig. 271) would suggest that these fibers may have this function. The Action of Drugs on the Inhibitory Apparatus. — The existence of the inhibitory fibers to the heart furnishes a means of explaining the cardiac action of a number of drugs, — atropin, muscarin or pilocarpin, nicotin, curare, digitalis, etc., — for the details of which reference must be made to works on pharmacology.* The action of the first three named illustrates especially well the application that has been made of physiology in modern pharma- cology. Atropin administered to those animals, such as the dog or man, in which the inhibitory fibers of the vagus are in constant activity, causes a quickening of the heart rate. Indeed, the heart beats as rapidly as if both vagi were cut. After the use of atropin, moreover, stimulation of the vagus nerve fails to produce inhibition. The action of atropin is satisfactorily explained by assuming that it paralyzes the endings of the (postganglionic) inhibitory fibers in the heart muscle, just as curare paralyzes the terminations of the motor fibers in skeletal muscle. Atropin exercises a similar effect upon the nerve terminations in the intrinsic muscles of the eyeball and in many of the glands. On the contrary, when mus- carin or pilocarpin is administered it causes a slowing and finally a cessation of the heart beat. Since this effect may be removed by the subsequent use of atropin it is assumed that the two former drugs excite or stimulate the endings of the inhibitory fibers in the heart and thus bring the organ to rest in diastole, as happens after electrical stimulation of the vagus nerve. Some authors, however, believe that the pilocarpin or muscarin may have a deeper effect in that it acts directly on the heart muscle itself, and that the antagonistic atropin affects the muscular tissue also as well as the endings of the fibers. A final statement can not be made upon this point, but the current belief is that the atropin paralyzes while the muscarin or pilocarpin stimulates the endings of the inhibitory fibers in the substance of the heart. The Nature of Inhibition. — Since the discovery of the inhibi- tory nerves of the heart furnished the first conclusive proof of the * Consult Cushnv, " Text-book of Pharmacology and Therapeutics." Philadelphia, 1903. 570 CIRCULATIOX OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. existence in the body of definite nerve fibers with apparently the sole function of inhibition, it seems appropriate in this connection to refer to the views regarding the nature of this process. Several general views of the nature of inhibition have been proposed, but the one that is most definite and has met with most favor is that suggested by Gaskell.* This author has shown that the after-effects of stimulation of the inhibitory fibers are beneficial rather than in- jurious to the heart ; that is, under certain circumstances an improve- ment may be noticed in the rate or force of the beat or in the con- ductivity. He has also shown, by an interesting experiment, that during the state of inhibition the heart tissue is made increasingly electropositive in comparison with a dead portion of the tissue. To show this fact the tip of the auricle was killed by heat and this spot (a) and a point at the base of the auricle (b) were connected with a galvanometer. Under such conditions a strong demarcation cur- rent was obtained flowing through the galvanometer from 6 to a. If the auricle contracted a negative variation resulted, since during activity b became less positive as regards a. If, on the contrary, the auricle was inhibited b}' stimulation of the inhibitory fibers a positive variation was obtained; b became more positive toward a. On the basis of such results Gaskell concludes that inhibition in the heart is due to a set of metabolic changes of an opposite character to those occurring during contraction. In the latter condition the metabolism is catabolic, and consists in the breaking down of complex substances into simpler ones with the liberation of energy as heat and work. During inhibition, on the contrary, the processes are anabolic or synthetic and result in the formation of increased contractile material whereby the condition of the heart is improved. He would regard the inhibitory fibers, therefore, as the anabolic nerve of the heart and their constant action throughout life as an aid to the nutrition of the heart. The same general view may be extended to all cases of inhibition, and Gaskell believes that all muscular tissues are supplied with anabolic (inhibitory) and catabolic (motor) fibers. f A more specific I heory applicable to the case of the hear! has been proposed by the author.}: In experiments made upon the isolated hearl of the ^ it has been shown thai during stimulation of the vagus potassium in diffusible form is given off from the hearl mu cle (auricles), it is known that potassium salt* in a certain concentration in the circulating Liquid will bring the heart to a stand-still, and the state of potassium inhibition thus produced resembles ♦Gaskell, "Philosophical Tran actions of the Royal Society," London; Croonian Lecture, pari m , L882; also "Beitrage zur Physiologic, " dedicated to C, Ludwig, L887; and "Journal of Physiology," 7, 46. f For a general discussion of iliis idea and of the Importance of inhibitory actions, see Meltzer, "Inhibition, " "New York Medical Journal," May i:;, 20, 27. L899. % Howell and Duke, "American Journal of Physiology," 21, 51, L908. THE CARDIAC NERVES. 571 very closely the state of vagus inhibition. Since the vagus when stimulated liberates potassium in a diffusible form, it is suggested that its action in stopping the heart is effected through the agency of this substance. The potassium exists in large percentage in the heart-muscle, but in a combined form, and the theory assumes that the vagus impulses initiate a dissociation or cleavage of some sort which sets free some potassium in soluble form. If it is assumed that this liberation takes place in the part of the heart in which the beat originates, the theory offers a simple explanation of the stoppage of the beat, of the quick recovery after stimulation ceases, and of the retention of irritability to direct stimula- tion shown by the heart during vagus inhibition. A heart that has been stopped by an excess of potassium chloride added to the circulating liquid beats very promptly as soon as the excess of the potassium is removed, and as in the case of vagus inhibition it seems often to show a notice- able improvement in condition. That the inhibitory ef- fect of the vagus im- pulses upon the heart is not due to any peculiarity in properties of these fibers or of the impulses themselves, but is depend- ent rather upon the place or manner of ending in the heart, has been demon- strated by direct experi- ment. Erlanger* has shown that when an ordinary spinal nerve (fifth cervical) is sutured to the peripheral end of the cut vagus, it will, after time for regeneration has been allowed, cause, when stimulated, the usual stoppage of the heart. The Course of the Ac- celerator Fibers. — The heart receives efferent or motor nerve fibers from the sympathetic system in addition to those reaching it by way of the vagus nerve. Atten- tion was first called to these sympathetic fibers by Legallois (1812), but our recent knowledge dates from the experiments made by von Bezold (1862), which were afterward completed * Erlanger, "American Journal of Physiology," 13, 372, 1905. Fig. 235. — Schematic representation of the course of the accelerator fibers to the dog's heart — right side. — (Modified from Pawlow.) The sym- pathetic nerve is represented in solid black. The course of the accelerator fibers is indicated by ar- rows. /, Cervical sympathetic combined in heck with, 10, the vagus; //, ///, IV, rami communi- cantes from the second, third, and fourth thoracic spinal nerves, carrying most of the accelerator fi- bers to the sympathetic chain ; 7, annulus of Vieus- sens; 8, inferior cervical ganglion; 2, 3, 4, 5, branches from vagus and vago-sympathetic trunk going to cardiac plexus (some of these — 3, 5, — carry accelerator fibers; 9, the inferior laryngeal nerve. 572 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. by the Cyon brothers— M. and E. Cyon* — 1866. These fibers when stimulated cause an increased rate of beat and are, there- fore, designated as the accelerator nerve of the heart. Their course has been worked out physiologically in a number of animals. Among the mammalia and, indeed, among different animals of the same species there is some variation, but a general conception of their origin and course may be obtained from Figs. 235 and 236, which represent in a schematic way the anatomical path taken by these fibers. They emerge from the spinal cord in the anterior roots of the second, third, and fourth thoracic spinal nerves. Accord- ing to some authors they may be found also in the fifth tho- racic, the first thoracic, or even the lower cervical spinal nerves. They pass then by way of the white rami to the stellate or first thoracic ganglion (6), and thence by way of the annulus of Vieussens (ansa subclavia) (7) to the inferior cervical ganglion. A number of branches leave the sympathetic system and the vagus in this region to pass to the cardiac plexus and thence to the heart. The accelerator fibers are found in some of these branches, mixed in some cases with inhibitory fibers from the vagus. In the cat Boehm has described a special branch (ner- vus accelerans) which runs from the stellate ganglion directly to the cardiac plexus (Fig. 236). The preganglionic portion of some of the accelerator fibers ends around the ganglion cells in the first thoracic ganglion, while others apparently make their first termination in the inferior cervical ganglion. The accelerator fibers may be stimulated in the -|)iual roots in which they emerge (II, III, IV), in the annulus, or in some of the branches that arise from the annulus, or from the inferior cervical ganglion (5, 3, 2). It will be borne in mind that no accelerator fibers are found in the cervical sympathetic above the inferior cervical ganglion. * For the hi tory and literal ure of the accelerator nerves, see ('yon, article "Cowir," p. L03, initichet's " Dictionnaire de Physiologie," L900; or Tiger- stedt, " Lehrbuch der Physiologie des Kreislaufes," 260, 1893. V\k- 236 — Sketch to show the accel- erator ::ni'i augmentor) branches from the stellate ganglion (in the cat, lefl side): L, The ven tr:il branch of the annulus (ansa subclavia); 2, small branch not constantly pre.-ent; :',, Bochm's accelerator nerve (N. canhacus c Kanglio stcllato.). THE CARDIAC NERVES. 573 At various times investigators have asserted that accelerator fibers are contained also in the vagus nerve. Thus, it has been shown that, after the paralysis of the inhibitory fibers in the heart by atropin, stimulation of the vagus causes an acceleration of the heart. Little attention has been paid to the physiology of these fibers, since it seems evident that the great outflow of accelerators is made via the sympathetic system. The Action of the Accelerator Fibers. — In experimental work the accelei*ators are usually stimulated in one or more of the branches represented schematically as 5, 3, 6, in Fig. 235, or 3, in Fig. 236. The effect is an increase in the rate of beat of the heart, which may be very evident, amounting to as much as 70 per cent. or more of the original rate, or may be very slight. When accelera- tion is obtained the latent period is considerable and the heart does not return at once to its normal rate upon cessation of the stimulus (see Fig. 237). In some cases the effect upon the heart is an acceleration pure and simple, — that is, the rate of beat is increased without any evidence of an increase in the force of the beats. The larger number of beats is offset by the smaller amplitude of each beat; so that the blood-pressure in the arteries is unchanged. In other cases the effect upon the heart may be an increase not only in rate but also in the force or amplitude of the beats, or the rate may remain unaffected and onty the amplitude of the heart beats be increased. For these reasons most authors favor the view that the accelerator nerves, so called, contain in reality two sets of fibers, one, the accelerators proper, whose function is simply to accelerate the rate, and one, the augmentors, that cause a more forcible beat. The augmenting action is obtained especially from the nerves of the left side. Tonicity of the Accelerators and Reflex Acceleration. — The results of the most careful work show, without doubt, that the accelerators to the heart are normally in a state of tonic activity.* When these nerves are cut upon both sides the heart rate is decreased . We must believe, therefore, that under normal conditions the heart muscle is under the constant influence of two antagonistic influ- ences, one through the inhibitory fibers tending to slow the rate, one through the accelerator fibers tending to quicken the rate. The actual rate at any moment is the resultant of these two influences. While such an arrangement seems at first sight to be unnecessary from a mechanical standpoint, it is doubtless true that it possesses some distinct advantage. Possibly it makes the heart more promptly responsive to reflex regulation. Balanced mechanisms of this kind are found in other parts of the body where smooth and prompt reactions to stimulation seem to be especially necessary, — ■ for example, the constrictor and dilator fibers of the iris, the ex- * For a discussion of this and other points in the physiology of the ac- celerators see Hunt, "American Journal of Phvsiology," 2, 395, 1S99, and "Journal of Experimental Medicine," 2, 151, 1897. 574 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. tensor and flexor muscles of the joints, etc. Physiologists have studied experimentally the effect upon the heart of stimulating simultaneously the inhibitory and the accelerator nerves. The work done upon this subject by Hunt seems to make it very certain that in all such cases the result, so far as the rate is concerned, is the algebraic sum of the effects of the separate stimulations of the nerve. The inhibitory and the accelerator fibers must be considered, therefore, as true antagonists, acting in opposite ways upon the heart. The existence of the accel- erator nerves makes possible, of course, their reflex stimulation. Experimentally it is found that stimulation of various sensory nerves — those of the limbs or trunk, for instance — may cause reflexly either an increase or decrease in the heart rate, and as a matter of experience we know that our heart rate may be increased by various changes, particularly by emotional states. The natural explanation of such accelerations is that they are Fig. 237. — To show the acceleration and augmentation produced by a strong stimulus. Isolated cat's heart, stimulation on left side. The upper curve gives the ventricular contractions, the lower one the auricular contractions. The lowermost line gives the time in seconds and the line above indicates the duration of the stimulation of the accel- erati r nerve. due to reflex stimulation of the nerve cells in the central nervous system which give rise to the accelerator fibers. But another point of view is possible. An increase in heart rate may be lnoiight about either by a reflex stimulation of the accelerator fibers or by a reflex inhibition of the cardio-inhibitory center. Hunt especially has presented many experimental facts which indicate; thai an increase in heart rate from reflex action may be produced by an inhibition of the tonic activity of the cardio- inhibitory center. He finds, for instance, that when the two \n 28; horse and ox, 36 50; sheep, 60-80; dog, 100-120; rabbit, 150; mice, 670 (Buchanan). It would seem, from these facts, that the f:i i rate in the small animals, with their shorter circulatory path and smaller volume of blood, is necessary to the mechanical fulfilment of the functions of the blood, and has been preserved by natural selection. 676 THE RATE OF THE HEART BEAT 577 Variations with Age. — In line with the last condition it is found in man that the pulse rate is highest in infancy, sinks quite rapidly at first and then more slowly up to adult life, and rises again slightly in very old age at the time that the body undergoes a perceptible shrinkage. The most extensive data upon this point are found in the works of the older observers.* According to Guy, a condensed summary of the average results obtained at different periods of life, both sexes included, may be given as follows : At birth 140 Infancy 120 Childhood 100 Youth 90 Adult age 75 Old age . . 70 Extreme age 75-80 The Variations in Pulse Rate Effected through the In- hibitory and Accelerator Nerves. — Most of the sudden adaptive changes of the heart rate come under this head. In the laboratory we find that stimulation of all sensory nerve trunks may affect the heart rate, in some cases increasing it, in others the reverse. In life we find that the pulse rate is very responsive to our changing sensations and especially to mental conditions that indicate deep interest or emotional excitement. In a previous paragraph (p. 573) the physiological cause of this effect has been discussed briefly. It may arise either from a reflex excitation of the accelera- tor nerves or a reflex inhibition of the tonic activit}^ of the inhibitory nerves. The facts at present seem to favor this latter explanation. In addition to these reflexes associated with conscious states the heart is susceptible to reflex influences of a totally unconscious character connected with the states of activity of the visceral organs. For example, after meals the heart beat increases usually in rate and especially in force of beat, thereby counteracting the effect on blood pressure of the large vascular dilatation in the intestinal area. Variations in Heart Rate ivith the Condition of Blood-pressure.- — It has long been known that when the blood-pressure in the arteries falls the pulse rate increases and when it rises the pulse rate de- creases. Thus, the low blood-pressure that is characteristic of the condition of surgical shock is associated with a very rapid rate of heart beat. There is a certain inverse relationship between pressure and rate which has the characteristics of a purposeful adaptation. The quicker pulse rate following upon the low pressure tends to increase the output of blood and raise the pressure. There was formerly much discussion as to whether this relationship is brought about by reflexes through the extrinsic nerves of the heart or whether it is due to some direct, perhaps mechanical, * See Volkmann, "Die Hamodynamik," p. 427, 1850; also Guy, article "Pulse" in Todd's "Cyclopaedia of Anatomy and Physiology," 1847-49. 37 57S CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. effect upon the heart. The experiments of Newell Martin upon the isolated heart seem to have settled the matter satisfactorily.* Bv a method devised by him he kept dogs' hearts beating for many hours when isolated from all connections with the body except the lungs. Under these conditions it was found that even extreme variations in blood-pressure did not affect the heart rate. Consequently, the variation that does take place under normal conditions must be due to a stimulation of the cardiac nerves. A rise of pressure in the arteries may affect directly the eardio-inhibitory center or it may affect afferent fibers in the heart or arteries, and thus reflexly stimulate the eardio-inhibitory center. This point has been the subject of a number of investigations, but Eyster and Hookerf appear to have demonstrated that both methods of stimulation occur. High arterial pressure affects the medullary center directly and thus slows the rate, but it affects also certain sensory fibers in the aorta at or beyond the arch, and through them causes a reflex slowing. Variations with Muscular Exercise. — It is a matter of everyday experience that the heart rate increases with muscular exercise. A simple change in posture, in fact, suffices to affect the heart rate. The rate is higher when standing (80) than when sitting (70) and higher in this latter condition than when lying down (66). Unusual exertion, as in running, causes a very marked and long-lasting increase in the pulse rate. The purposeful charac- ter of this adaptation is very evident. Increase in muscular activity calls for a more rapid circulation to supply the oxygen and other elements of nutrition, but the physiological mechanism by which this adaptation is obtained is not explained satisfactorily. Johans- son,} who has studied the matter carefully, concludes that the effect is due mainly to two causes: First, to the effect of the chem- ical products of metabolism in the active muscle, which are given off to the circulation and are then carried to the nerve centers where they affect the cardiac nerves, or possibly to an effect of these metabolic products on the heart directly. Ho considers this factor as of relatively subordinate importance. Second, the chief factor is found in an associated activity of the accelerator nerves. Thai is, the discharge of impulses along the voluntary motor paths (pyramidal) sets into activity at the same time and proportionally the center of the accelerator nerve fibers. Hering§ supports the latter pari of this explanation to the extent- of showing that the increase in heart rate after mUSCUlar exercise is dependent * Martin, "Studies from the Biological Laboratory, Johns Hopkins Uni- versity," 2, 213, 1882; also "Collected Phy iologica] Papers," p. '2.',, 1896. t Eyster and Hooker, "American Journal of Physiology, " 21, 373, l(.)()8. J Johann on, "Skanoinavisches lArchiv f. Physiologic, " 5, 2Q, L896. g "Centralblatl f. Phyi iologie," 8, 75, L894. THE RATE OF THE HEART BEAT. 579 upon the integrity of the accelerator nerves. On the other hand, after prolonged or excessive muscular exertion the heart rate remains accelerated for a considerable period after cessation of the work — in the untrained individual at least — a fact which would indicate some long-lasting influence, such as is implied in the first factor given above, namely, the effect of the products of muscular metabolism. Variations with the Gaseous Conditions of the Blood. — In con- ditions of asphyxia the altered gaseous contents of the blood increase in C02 and decrease in 02, act upon the medullary centers of the cardiac nerves, causing, first, an increase and then a decrease in heart rate. The Variations in Pulse Rate Due to Changes in the Composi- tion or Properties of the Blood. — The condition under this head that has the most marked influence upon the heart rate is the temperature of the blood. Speaking generally, the rate of beat increases regularly with the temperature of the blood or other circulating liquid up to a cer- tain optimum temperature. On the heart of the cold-blooded animal this relationship is easily demonstrated by supplying the heart with an arti- ficial circulation of Ringer's solu- tion, which can be heated or cooled at pleasure. The rate and force of the beat increase to a maximum, which is reached at about 30° C. (see Fig. 238). Beyond this opti- mum temperature the beats decrease in force and also in rate, becoming irregular or fibrillar before the heart finally comes to rest. Newell Mar- tin* has shown the same relation- ship in a very conclusive way upon the isolated heart of the dog. Within physiological limits the rate of beat rises and falls substantially parallel to the variations in temperature as is shown by the chart reproduced in Fig. 239. The accelerated heart rate in fevers is therefore due probably to the * Martin, " Croonian Lecture, Philosophical Transactions, Roval Society," London, 174, 663, 1883; also "Collected Physiological Papers," p. 40, 1895. Fig. 238.— To show the effect of temperature on the rate and force of the heart beat. Contractions of the terrapin's ventricle at different tem- peratures. Kymograph moving at the same speed. At 30° the rate is still increasing, but the extent of con- traction has passed its optimum. 580 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AXD LYMPH. direct influence of the high temperature upon the heart itself. The same observer determined experimentally the upper and lower lethal limits of temperature for the mammalian heart. The experi- ments were made upon cats' hearts kept alive by artificial circu- lation through the coronary arteries.* It was found that the high- est temperature at which the heart will beat is about 44° to 45° C, It- sir § 3 k | « K ^ t-ir V <^ u \ \ \ \ Si tz 6/ it > :-. X \ s \ / / / tl II 60 / ; /' :'' - ' ul iDy i 6i It \ «- (S It ^ \ \ \ / \ M i v • - t - \ ■ ! \\\ • i : 5; ■ i • > « \- i * i i > ■ ■ ■ j •• J' ; ; :i *: ■ ■* , J i • O.S '.H >_ tjja B.~ " S? - 0 g b£~ 0) £ ■s»a.aS ** 3 G B g.SE°g

fH £ 2 X « ?>S- C 4) °E, 3 <" » rt «H 5 c "• ^.J -=TJ Eg73! a tl /. _ • - Z ^ m£ ^ a I-88SB d « _ r "55 O - a * is tn O . o 9 b^Hjs 3.H ■ ■•» -o W . 0) g hi i jSfcJ ills although a alightly higher temperature may be withstood under special conditions. At the other extreme the mammalian heart ceases to beal when the temperature fulls as low as L70tol8°C. 'I he rate of the hearl beal may bo influenced also by many sub- stances added to I he blood. The influence of atropin and muscarin • Martin and A-pplegarth, "Studies from the Biological Laboratory, Johm Hopkins I oiver ity," 4, 275, 1890; also "Collected Physiological Papers," p. 97, L895. THE RATE OF THE HEART BEAT. 581 has already been alluded to, but changes also in the normal con- stituents of the blood may have similar effects. Thus, an increase in the sodium carbonate of the blood affects the heart beat, particu- larly in regard to the amplitude or force of the contraction, while variations in the other inorganic constitutents may have a marked influence on the rate. The most significant and strik- ing fact in this connection is the relation of the potassium salts. As the amount of diffusible potassium is increased the pulse rate becomes slower and slower, until the heart stops in a con- dition of potassium inhibition. CHAPTER XXXII. THE VASOMOTOR NERVES AND THEIR PHYSIO- LOGICAL ACTIVITY. During the first half of the nineteenth century the physical or mechanical conditions of the circulation were carefully studied and great emphasis was laid upon such properties as the elasticity of the coats of the vessels. The physical adaptability thereby con- ferred upon the vascular tubes was thought to be sufficient for the purposes of the circulation. We now know that many of the blood- vessels are supplied with motor and inhibitory nerve fibers through whose activity the size of the vascular bed and the distribution of blood to the various organs are regulated. We know, also, that without this nervous control the vascular system fails entirely to meet what seems to be the most important condition of a normal circulation, — namely, the maintenance of a high arterial pressure. Although a number of physiologists had assumed the existence of nerve fibers capable of acting upon the muscular coats of the blood- vessels, the experimental proof of the existence of such nerves, and the beginning of the modern development of the theory of vasomotor regulation were a part of the brilliant contributions to physiology made by Claude Bernard.* In 1851 Bernard discovered that when the sympathetic nerve is cut in the neck of a rabbit the blood-vessels in the ear on the same side become very much dilated. He and other observers afterward showed that if the peripheral (head) end of the served nerve is stimulated electrically the ear becomes blanched, owing to a constriction of the blood-vessels. Thus the existence of vasoconstrictor nerve fibers to the blood-vessels was demonstrated. A vast amount of experimental work has been done since to ascertain the exact distribution of these fibers to the various organs and the reflex conditions under which they function normally. Few subjects in physiology are of more practical im- portance to the physician than that of vasomotor regulation; it plays such a large and constant part in the normal activity of the various organs. Bernard was doubly fortunate in being the first fcO demonstrate the existence of a second class of nerve; filters, which. when stimulated, cause a dilatation of the blood-vessels and which * See "Life of Claude Bernard," bySir Michael Poster, 1899, in the series, "Masters of Medicine." 582 THE VASOMOTOR NERVES. 583 are therefore designated as vasodilator nerve fibers. This discovery was made in connection with the chorda tympani nerve, a branch of the facial, which sends secretory fibers to the submaxillary gland. When this nerve is cut and the peripheral end is stimulated a secre- tion of saliva results and at the same time, as Bernard showed, the blood-vessels of the gland dilate; the flow of blood is greatly in- creased in the efferent vein and may even show a pulse. In the nervous regulation of the blood-vessels we have to con- sider, therefore, the existence and physiological activities of two antagonistic sets of nerve fibers: First, the vasoconstrictor fibers, whose action causes a contraction of the muscular coats of the ar- teries and therefore a diminution in the size of the vessels. Second, the vasodilator nerve fibers, whose action causes an increase in size of the blood-vessels, due probably to a relaxation (inhibition) of the muscular coats of the arteries. Before attempting to describe the present state of our knowledge upon these points it will be help- ful to refer to some of the methods by means of which the existence of vasomotor fibers has been demonstrated. Methods Used to Determine Vasomotor Action. — The simplest and most direct proof is obtained from mere inspection, when this is possible. If stimulation of the nerve to an organ causes it to blanch, the presence of vasoconstrictor fibers is dem- onstrated unless the organ is muscular and the blanching may be regarded as a mechanical result. On the other hand, if stimulation of the nerve to an organ causes it to become congested or flushed with blood the presence of vasodilator fibers may be accepted. It is obvious, however, that this method is applicable in only a few instances and that in no case does it lend itself to quantitative study. 2. Vasomotor effects may be determined by measur- ing the outflow of blood from the veins. If stimulation of the nerve to an organ causes a decrease in the flow of blood from the veins of that organ, this fact implies the existence of vasoconstrictor fibers, while an opposite result indicates vasodilator fibers. 3. By variations in arterial and venous pressures. When vaso- constrictor fibers are stimulated there is a rise of pressure in the artery supplying the organ and a fall of pressure in the veins emerging from the organ. This result is what we should expect if the constriction takes place in the region of the arterioles. The diminution in size of these vessels by increasing peripheral resistance augments the internal pressure on the arterial side of the resistance, and causes a fall of side pressure on the venous side (see p. 497). If the area involved is large enough the increased resistance will make a perceptible difference in pressure, not only in the organ supplied, but also in the aorta; there will be a rise of general (dias- tolic) blood-pressure. On the other hand, a vasodilator action in 584 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. any organ is accompanied by the reverse changes. Peripheral resistance being diminished there will be a fall of pressure on the arterial side and a rise of pressure on the venous side. When, therefore, the stimulation of any nerve brings about a rise of arterial pressure that can not be referred to a change in the heart beat the inference made is that the result is clue to a vasocon- striction. When the method is applied to a definite organ — the brain, for instance — it becomes conclusive only when simultaneous observations are made upon the pressure in the artery and the vein of the organ, and proof is obtained that the pressures at these points vary in opposite directions. 4. By observations upon the volume of the organ. It is obvious that, other conditions remaining un- changed, a vasoconstriction in an organ will be accompanied by a diminution in volume, and a vasodilatation by an increase in volume. This method of studying the blood-supply of an organ is designated as plethysmography, and any instrument designed to record the changes in volume of an organ is a plethysmograph* Plethysmography have been designed for special organs, and in such cases they have sometimes been given special names. Thus, the plethysmograph used upon the kidney and spleen has been desig- nated as an oncometer, that for the heart, as a cardiometer. The precise form and structure of a plethysmograph varies, of course, with the organ studied, but the principle used is the same in all cases. The organ is inclosed in a box with rigid walls that have an opening at some one point only, and this opening is placed in connection with a recorder of some kind by tubing with rigid walls. The connections between recorder and plethysmograph and the space in the interior of the latter not occupied by the organ may be filled with air or, as is more usually the case, with water. The idea of a plethysmograph may be illustrated by the skull. This structure forms a natural pelthysmograph for the brain. If a hole is bored through the skull at any point and a connection is then made with a recorder of some kind, such as a tambour, the volume changes of the brain may be registered successfully. The plethysmograph generally employed in laboratories, particularly for in- vestigations on man, is some modification of the form devised by Mosso (see Fig. 240). The hand and more or lees of the arm is placed in a glass cylinder whirl, Lb swung freel) from a support. The opening around the arm is shut off by a cuff of rubber dam that must be chosen of such a size as to fit the arm snugly without compression of the superficial veins. The forward end of the plethysmograph is connected by tubing with a re- corder, Through appropriate openings ihe cylinder and connecting tubes arc filled with warm water and then all openings arc closed except, the one leading to the recorder. \ny increase in volume of the arm will drive water from the plethj Olograph to the recorder, :unl any decrease;, on * for a description of I he development of I his method, see hrancois-Franck- Marey's " Travaux du Laboratoire, L876, p. I. THE VASOMOTOR NERVES. 585 the contrary, will suck water from the recorder into the plethysmograph. In the author's laboratory a modification that has been found most conve- nient is represented in Fig. 241. To avoid escape of water at the upper end of the tube and at the same time to prevent compression of the veins of the arm a very thin rubber glove with long gauntlet is used. The gauntlet is strengthened by cuffs of dam tubing, as shown in the illustration, and all are reflected over the end of the plethysmograph. The outer cuff (3) may be omitted. The hand is inserted into the cylinder and is held in place by flexing the fingers through the rings. The plethysmograph being suspended freely from the ceiling, any movement of the arm will move the instrument as a whole without disturbing the position of the arm in the instrument. By means of rings of hard rubber (D,E), one fitting around the rim of the plethys- mograph and the other adapted more or less closely to the size of the forearm, the reflected portion of the gauntlet and cuff is held in place and prevented from giving way readily to any rise of pressure in the plethysmograph. The Fig. 240. — A schematic diagram of Mosso's plethysmograph for the arms: a, the glass cylinder for the arm, with rubber sleeve and two tubulatures for filling with warm water; s, the spiral spring swinging the test tube, t. The spring is so calibrated that the level of the liquid in the test tube above the arm remains unchanged as the tube is filled and emptied. The movements of the tube are recorded on a drum by the writing point, 5. interior of the latter is connected, as shown in Fig. 240, to a test tube swung by a spiral spring (Bowditch's recorder) . The spring is so adjusted by trial that it sinks and rises exactly in proportion to the inflow or outflow of water. By this means the level of the water in the tube is kept constant, and since the posi- tion of this level determines the pressure upon the outside of the arm in the plethysmograph this pressure is also kept constant independently of the changes hi volume of the arm. The level should be set in the beginning so as to make a slight positive pressure on the arm sufficient to flatten the thin glove to the skin and thus drive out the air between the two. When the apparatus is conveniently arranged, with slings to support the elbow, observations may be made upon the changes in volume of the arm during long periods. The results so obtained are referred to under several headings. With the form of recorder described the plethysmograph gives usually only the slow changes in volume of the arm, clue to a greater or less amount of blood. Bv using a more sensitive recorder and making the con- 586 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH nections entirely rigid the smaller, quicker changes in volume caused by the heart beat are also recorded. A volume pulse is obtained resembling in its general form the pressure pulse given by the sphygmograph. When used for this purpose the instrument is described as a hydrosphygmograph. Records taken of the volume of the hand, foot, brain, or any other organ show that in addition to the changes caused by the heart beat and by the respiratory movements, there are other more irregular variations that are continually occurring the cause of which is to be found in the variations in the amount of blood in the organ. Day and night these changes in volume take place, and they are referable to the activity of the vasomotor system. Vasoconstriction or vasodilatation in the organ itself cause what may be called Fig. "41. — Detailed drawing of the glass plethysmograph with the arrangement of rub- ber glove to prevent leaking without compressing the veins. 2, The glove with its gauntlet reflected over the end of the glass cylinder; 1 and .i, supporting pieces ot stout rubber tub- ing; D and E, sections of outer and inner rings of hard rubber to fasten the reflected rubber tubing and reduce the opening for the arm. an active change in volume. But vasoconstriction or vasodilata- tion in other organs may cause a perceptible change, of a passive kind, in the volume of the organ under observation. For, since the amount of blood remains the same, a change in any one organ musl affect more or less the volume — that is, the blood contents — of all other organs. General Distribution and Course of the Vasoconstrictor Nerve Fibers. — These fibers belong to the autonomic system, and consist, therefore, of a preganglionic fiber arising in the central nervous system and a postganglionic fiber arising from the cell of some sympathetic ganglion. The general arrangement of the auto- nomic system (p. 246) should be reviewed in this connection. It has been shown by experiments of the kind described under the last heading that vasoconstrictor fibers are present in numerous nerve THE VASOMOTOR NERVES. 587 trunks, but especially in those distributed to the skin and to the abdominal and pelvic organs. If, for instance, the sciatic or the splanchnic nerve be cut, to avoid reflex effects, and the peripheral end be stimulated, there will be a strong constriction of the vessels, which may be detected by ocular inspection, blanching; by the increase in arterial pressure; or by the diminution in volume of the organs. The vasoconstrictor fibers supplying these two great regions arise immediately (postganglionic fibers) from one or other of the ganglia constituting the sympathetic chain, or from the large prevertebral ganglia (celiac ganglion, for instance) directly con- nected with it. Ultimately, of course, they arise in the central nervous system (preganglionic fiber), and it has been shown that, for the regions under consideration, they all, with a few compara- tively unimportant exceptions, leave the spinal cord in the great Fig. 242. —Schema to show the path of the preganglionic and postganglionic portions of a vasoconstrictor nerve fiber: a, Anterior root, showing the course of the preganglionic fiber as a dotted line ; d, v, dorsal and ventral branches of the spinal nerve ; r, the ramus communicans; g, the sympathetic ganglion. The postganglionic fibers in each ramus come from the sympathetic ganglion with which it is connected. The preganglionic fibers enter- ing at any ganglion may pass up or down to end in the cells of some other ganglion. outflow that takes place in the thoracic region from the second thoracic to the second lumbar nerves (p. 248). In this outflow they are mixed with other autonomic fibers, such as the sweat fibers, pilomotor fibers, accelerator fibers to heart, pupilodilator fibers, visceromotor fibers, etc. Emerging in the anterior roots, they pass to the sympathetic chain by way of the corresponding ramus communicans. Having reached the chain, they end in one or other of the ganglia, not necessarily in the ganglion with which the ramus connects anatomically. The preganglionic fibers for the blood- vessels of the submaxillary gland, for instance, enter the first thoracic ganglion of the sympathetic chain, but do not actually terminate until they reach the superior cervical ganglion high in the neck. The postganglionic fibers arise in the ganglion in which the 5SS CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. preganglionic fibers terminate. Those destined to supply the skin of the trunk and extremities pass from the ganglion to the cor- responding spinal nerve by way of the ramus communicans (gray ramus) and after reaching the spinal nerve they are distributed with it to its corresponding region (Fig. 242). In the general region Fig. 243. — Vasomotor effect of stimulation of the spl;inelmi<- nerve — peripheral end — hi the dog (Dawson): I. The line of zero pressure ; 2, the line of the timulating pen ; on and off mark the beginning and end of the stimulation; •''., the time record in seconds; 4, the blood-pre ure record ' limnlntioii ran >-.-. a mucked rise of blood-pressure due to stimu- lation of vb ocon trictoi fibei ); 5, plethysmographic tracing of the volume ol the kidney (oncometer); timulation of the planchnic causes a diminution in volume of the kidney owing to the con triction of i' arteriole under consideration (lower cervical to upper lumbar) each ramus communicans between a spinal nerve and a sympathetic ganglion consists, therefore, of two parts, one (white ramus) of preganglionic fibers passing from the spinal nerve to the ganglion, the older ramus) of postganglionic fibers coining from the ganglion to THE VASOMOTOR NERVES. 589 the spinal nerve for distribution to the peripheral tissues. It should be borne in mind that the fibers in the white ramus do not return to the spinal nerve by the gray portion of the same ramus, but passing upward or downward in the sympathetic chain return to some other spinal nerve as postganglionic fibers. In this way, therefore, it happens that the various intercostal nerves and the nerves of the brachial and sciatic plexus contain vasoconstrictor fibers as postganglionic or sjmipathetic fibers. On the other hand, the vasoconstrictor fibers destined for the great vascular region of the intestines and other abdominal viscera, after reaching the sym- pathetic chain by way of the white rami as preganglionic fibers, do not return to the spinal nerves by the gray rami. They leave the sympathetic chain, still as preganglionic fibers, in the branches of the splanchnic nerves and through them pass to the celiac ganglion, where they mainly end, and their path is continued by the post- ganglionic or sympathetic fibers arising from this ganglion. More specific information concerning the origin of the vasomotor fibers to the different organs is given in condensed form farther on. It is quite important in the beginning, however, to obtain a clear general conception of the paths taken by the constrictor fibers from their origin in the spinal cord to their termination, on the one hand, in the vessels of the skin, or, on the other, in the vessels of the abdominal and pelvic viscera. The Tonic Activity of the Vasoconstrictor Fibers. — A very important fact regarding the vasoconstrictor nerve fibers is that they are constantly in action to a greater or less extent. This fact is demonstrated by the simple experiment of cutting them. If the sympathetic nerve in the neck is cut in the rabbit the blood- vessels of the ear become dilated. If the splanchnic nerves on the two sides are cut the intestinal region becomes congested, and the effect in this case is so great that the general arterial pressure falls to a very low point. From these and numerous similar ex- periments we may conclude that normally the arteries — that is, the arterioles — are kept in a condition of tone by impulses received through the vasoconstrictor fibers. Cut these nerves and the arte- ries lose their tone and dilate, with the result that, the peripheral resistance being diminished, the lateral pressure falls on the arterial side and rises on the venous side. The relatively enormous effect upon aortic pressure caused by paralysis of the tone of the arteries in the splanchnic area shows that under normal conditions the peripheral resistance in this great area plays a predominating part in the maintenance of normal arterial pressure, and by the same reasoning variations in tone in the arteries of this region must play a very large part in the regulation of arterial pressure. The Vasoconstrictor Center. — As stated in the last two para- 590 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. graphs, the vasoconstrictor fibers emerge from the cord over a definite region, and they exhibit constant tonic activity. It has been shown, moreover, that if the cord be cut anywhere in the cervical region all of the constrictor fibers lose their tone; a great vascular dilatation results in both the splanchnic and skin areas. We may infer from this fact that the vasoconstrictor paths originate from nerve cells in the brain and that their tonic activity is to be traced to these cells. Such a group of cells exists in the medulla oblongata, and forms the vaso- constrictor center. The axons given off from these cells de- scend in the cervical cord and terminate at various levels in the anterior horn of gray mat- ter in the region from the upper thoracic to the upper lumbar spinal nerves. A spinal neuron continues the path as the pre- ganglionic vasoconstrictor fiber which terminates, as already described, in some sympathetic ganglion, whence the path is further continued by the post- ganglionic fiber. This arrange- ment of the constrictor paths is indicated schematically in Fig. 244. The exact location of the group of cells that plays the im- portant role of a vasoconstrictor center has not been determined histologically. The region has, however, been delimited roughly by physiological experiments. If the brain is cut through at the level of the midbrain there is no marked loss of vascular tone in the body sA large. II'. however, similar sections are made farther and farther back a point is reached al which vascular paralysis begins to be apparent and a point farther down at which this paralysis is as complete as it would be if the cervical cord were cut. Between these two points tin- vasoconstrictor center must lie. The careful experiments of this kind made by Dittmar* are now somewhat old. According * " Berichte d. Sflch . Ikademie, Math.-phys. EClasse," L873, p, 449. Fin. 244. — Schema to show the path of tin- vaaocoii-tiictoi IiIm-i- from the vaso- constrictor center to tin' blood-vessels and ecbanism for the reflex stimulation of these fibers: v. <■.. The vasoconstrictor er-nter; 1, the eent r;il neuron of the vaso- oonstnetor path; 2, the spinal neuron (preganglionic fiber); '■'•. the sympathetic neuron (postganglionic fiber); ", the arte- riole; i, the sensory fibers of the p., terior root making connections by collaterals with thl i rietor renter ; 5, an in- tercentral lil>er (efferent) acting upon the .a ocon i rietor center. THE VASOMOTOR NERVES. 591 to his description, the center is bilateral, — that is, consists of a group of cells on each side, — and lies about the middle of the fourth ventricle in the tegmental region, in the neighborhood of the nucleus of the facial and of the superior olivary. In the rabbit it has a length of 3 mms., a breadth of 1 to 1.5 mms., and lies about 2 to 2.5 mms. lateral to the mid-line. Assuming the existence of this group of cells, we must attribute to them functions of the first importance. Like other motor cells, they are capable of being stimulated reflexly and by this means the regulation of the blood- flow is largely controlled. Moreover, they are in constant activity, — due doubtless also to a constant reflex stimulus from the inflow of afferent impulses. The complete loss of this tonic influence would result in a complete vascular paralysis, the small arteries would be dilated, peripheral resistance would be greatly diminished, and the arterial pressure in the aorta would fall from a level of 100-150 mms. Hg to about 20 or 30 mms. Hg, — a pressure insuffi- cient to maintain the life of the organism. There seems to be no question now that in those conditions known as surgical shock the loss of control by the vasomotor center, and the consequent vascular paralysis and fall of blood-pressure, are the chief conditions of a serious character. We must conceive, also, that in this vasocon- strictor center the different cells are connected by definite paths with the vasoconstrictor fibers to the different regions of the body ; that some of the cells, for instance, control the activity of the fibers distributed to the intestinal area, and others govern the vessels of the skin. Under physiological conditions the different parts of the center may, of course, be acted upon separately. Vasoconstrictor Reflexes — Pressor and Depressor Nerve Fibers. — It is obvious that such a mechanism as that described above is susceptible of reflex stimulation through sensory nerves, and according to our general knowledge we should suppose that a tonic center of this kind may have its tonicity increased (excita- tion) or decreased (inhibition). Numerous experiments in phys- iology warrant the view that both kinds of effects take place normally. Those afferent nerve fibers which when stimulated cause reflexly an excitation of the vasoconstrictor center, and therefore a peripheral vasoconstriction and rise of arterial pressure, are frequently designated as pressor fibers, or their effect upon the circulation is designated as a pressor effect. Those afferent fibers, on the contrary, which when stimulated cause a diminution in the tone of the vasoconstrictor center and therefore a periph- eral vasodilatation and fall of arterial pressure, are designated as depressor nerve fibers, or their effect upon the circulation is a de- pressor effect. Pressor effects may be obtained by stimulation of almost any of the large nerves containing afferent fibers, but espe- 592 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. cially perhaps of the cutaneous nerves. And there is abundance of evidence to show that similar results can be obtained in man. The pressor effect manifests itself by a rise in general arterial pres- sure, if a sufficiently large region is involved, and by a diminution in size of the organ involved. On the other hand, depressor effects may also be obtained from stimulation of many of the large nerve trunks. If one stimulates the central end of the sciatic nerve, for example, one obtains a pressor effect on the circulation in most cases, but under certain conditions a marked depressor effect fol- lows the stimulation.* The simplest explanation of such a result is that the nerve trunks contain afferent fibers of both kinds. When we apply our electrodes to a nerve we stimulate every fiber in it and the actual result will depend upon which group of fibers exerts the stronger action, and this may vary with the condition litr. -'•"' i -rapine curve of forearm. The volume of the arm was recorded by mean of a counter-weighted tambour and the record .shows the pulse waves. A problem in mental arithmetic- the product of 24 by 43 — caused a marked constriction of the arm. of the nerve, the condition of the center, the anesthetic used, etc. Under normal conditions no such gross stimulation occurs. The pressor fibers arc stimulated under some circumstances, the de- pressor fibers under others. For instance, when the skin is exposed bo cold it is blanched not by a direct, but by a reflex, effect. The low temperature Stimulates (lie sensory (cold) libers in the skin, and the nerve impulses thus aroused reflexly stimulate the vaso- constrietor center, or ;i pari of it,, and cause blanching of the skin. -iirc to high temperatures, on the contrary, Hushes the skin, and in this case we may suppose thai the sensory impulses carried by the heal nerves inhibit the tone of the vasoconstrictor center and cause dilatation or flushing of Hie skin. So far as man is concerned, experiments made with the plethysmograph show very e Hunt, "Journal of Physiology," 18,381, 1895. THE VASOMOTOR NERVES. 593 clearly that the vasoconstrictor center is easily affected in a pressor or depressor manner by psychical states* or activities. Mental work, especially mental interest, however aroused, is followed by a constriction of the blood-vessels of the skin, — a pressor effect (see Fig. 245) ; and we may find an explanation of the value of the reflex in the supposition that the rise of arterial pressure thus produced Fig. 246.— Effect of stimulating the central end of the depressor nerve of the heart in a rabbit. The time record marks seconds. On and off mark the beginning and end of the stimulation. The blood-pressure rises slowly after the removal of the stimulus and eventually reaches the normal level. This complete recovery is not shown in the portion of the record reproduced. (Daivson.) forces more blood through the brain (p. 608). On the other hand, feelings of embarrassment or shame may be associated with a de- pressor effect, a dilatation in the vessels of the skin manifested, for example, in the act of blushing. In both cases we must assume intracentral nerve paths between the cortex and the center in the medulla, the impulses along one path exciting the center, while those along the other inhibit its tone, or, as explained below, excite 594 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. a vasodilator center. Among the many depressor effects that have been observed on' stimulation of afferent nerve fibers one has amused especial interest — namely, that caused by certain afferent fibers from the heart or from the aorta. So far as the effect in question is concerned the physiological evidence indi- cates that the fibers arise from the descending aorta ami it might be more appropriate to speak of them as the depressor nerve of the aorta.* These fibers in some animals — the dog, for instance — run in the vagus nerve, but in other animals, the rabbit, they form a separate nerve, the so-called depressor nerve of the heart — discovered by Ludwig ami ('yon (1866). In the rabbit this nerve forms a branch of the vagus, arising high in the neck by two roots, one from the trunk of the vagus and one from the superior laryngeal branch. It runs toward the heart in the sheath with the vagus and the cervical sympa- thetic. The nerve is entirely afferent. If it is cut and the peripheral end is stimulated no result follows. If, however, the central end is stimulated a fall of blood-pressure occurs and also perhaps a slowing of the heart beat (see Fig. 246). The latter effect is due to a reflex: stimulation of the cardio-inhibitory center and may be eliminated by previous section of the vagus. The fall of blood-pressure is explained by supposing that the nerve, when stimulated, inhibits, to a greater or less extent, the tonic activ- ity of the vasoconstrictor center. f Physiological experiments indicate that the nerve plays an important regulatory role.J When, for instance, blood-pressure rises above normal limits, it may be supposed that the endings of this nerve in the aorta or heart are stimulated by the mechanical effect, and the blood- pressure is thereby lowered by an inhibition of t he I one of the con- Btrictor center. Moreover, it has been shown by Einthoven I ha1 every heart beat sends up this nerve a series of nerve impulses, that is, when the nerve is cut and the ends are connected with a si ring-galvanometer, electrical variations occur synchronous with the heartbeat (Fig. 271). To explain this result we can on];.- assume that each heart beat stimulates sensory endings in the heart itself or in the aorta, and that- the nerve impulses thus transmitted to the medulla probably play a role in main- taining the tonic activity of some of its centers, perhaps, as Einthoven suggests, the Ionic activity especially of the cardio- inhibitor) center. 1908 * See Ey fcer and Hooker, "American Journal of Physiology, " 21, '■'>"'■'>, 'us. ,ii o fcfl ter and Tschermak, "Archiv 1'. die gesammte Physiologic, " !, 24, L902. f See Porter and Beyer, ''American Journal of Physiology, " 4, 283, 1900; ; : . Bayli , Journal of Phy iology," I I, 303, 1893. % Sewall and Steiner, "Journal of Physiology," 6, L62, L885. THE VASOMOTOR NERVES. 595 A most suggestive example of the regulating action of the depressor nerve is given by Sewall. When the carotids in a rabbit are clamped a variable and not very large rise of arterial pressure is observed. If, however, the depressor nerves are first cut, clamping the carotids causes an extraordinary rise of arterial pressure. When the carotids are closed we may suppose that the resulting anemia of the medulla stimulates the vasoconstrictor center and thus tends to raise arterial pressure, but this effect is neutralized because as the pressure rises the depressor fibers of the heart are stimulated. It seems evident that during life the depressor fibers must exert a very important regulating effect upon the circulation. A similar nerve has been described anatomically in man, while in animals like the dog, in which it is not present as a separate anatomical structure, it probably exists within the trunk of the vagus. If this latter nerve is cut in the dog and the central end is stimulated a depressor effect is usually obtained. Vasoconstrictor Centers in the Spinal Cord. — From the description of the vasoconstrictor mechanism given above the probable inference may be made that throughout the thoracic region the cells of origin of the preganglionic fibers may, under special conditions, act as subordinate vasoconstrictor centers capable of giving reflexes and of exhibiting some tonic activity. Numerous experiments tend to support this inference. When the spinal cord is cut in the lower thoracic region there is a paralysis of vascular tone in the posterior extremities. If, however, the animal is kept alive the vessels gradually re- cover their tone, although not connected with the medullary center. The re- sumption of tone in this case may be attributed to the nerve cells in the lower thoracic and upper lumbar region, since vascular paralysis is again produced when this portion of the cord is destroyed. Finally, Goltz has shown that when the entire cord is destroyed, except the cervical region (p. 155), vascular tone may be restored finally in the blood-vessels affected. In this case the re- sumption of tonicity must be referred either to the properties of the muscular coats of the arteries themselves reacting to the stimulus of the internal pres- sure, or to the activity of the sympathetic nerve cells that give rise to the postganglionic fibers. Under normal conditions it seems quite clear that the great vasoconstrictor center in the medulla is the important seat of tonic and of reflex activity. If the connections of this center with the blood-vessels are destroyed suddenly — for example, by cutting the cervical cord — blood-pressure falls at once to such a low level, 20 to 30 mms. Hg., that death usually results unless artificial means are employed to sustain the animal. Rhythmical Activity of the Vasoconstrictor Center. — Throughout life the vasoconstrictor center is in tone the intensity of which varies with the intensity and character of the reflex im- pulses playing upon it. Under certain unusual conditions the center may exhibit rhythmical variations in tonicity which make themselves visible as rhythmical rises and falls in the general arterial pressure (Fig. 247), the waves being much longer than those due to the respiratory movements. These waves of blood- pressure- are observed often in experiments upon animals, but their ultimate cause is not understood. They are usually desig- nated as Traube-Hering waves, although this term, strictly speak- ing, belongs to waves, synchronous with the respiratory move- ments, that were observed by Traube upon animals in which the diaphragm was paralyzed and the thorax was opened. These latter waves are also due to a rhythmical action of the 596 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. vasomotor center. During sleep, certain much longer, wave-like variations in the blood-pressure also occur that are again due doubt- less to a rhvthmical change of tone in the vasoconstrictor center. /^x y**\ y/vx y*"%< s \s \^r v/ Fig. 247. — Rhythmical vasomotor waves of blood-pressure in a dog (Traube-Hering waves). The upper tracing (1) is the blood-pressure record as taken with the mercury manometer; the lower tracing (2) is taken with a Hiirthle manometer. Seven distinct respiratory waves of blood-pressure may be recognized on each large wave. {Dawson.) General Course and Distribution of the Vasodilator Fibers. — By definition a vasodilator fiber is an efferent fiber which when stimulated causes a dilatation of the arteries in the region supplied. In searching for the existence of such fibers in the various nerve trunks physiologists have used all the methods referred to above, — namely, the flushing of the organ as seen by the eye, the increased blood-flow, the increase in volume, or the fall in blood-pressure on the arterial side associated with a rise on the venous side. By these methods vasodilator fibers have been demonstrated in the following regions: 1. In the facial nerve. The dilator fibers are found in the chorda tym- pani branch and arc distributed to the salivary glands (submaxil- lary and sublingual) and to the anterior two-thirds of the tongue. 2. In the glossopharyngeal nerve. Supplies dilator fibers to the posterior third of tongue, tonsils, pharynx, parotid gland (tympanic nerve). '.',. In the sympathetic chain. In the cervical portion of the sympathetic dilator fibers are carried which are distributed to the mucous mem- brane of the moutli (lips, gums, and palate), nostrils, and the skin Of the cheeks. These libers pass up the neck to (lie superior cervi- ca] ganglion and thence by communicating branches reach the (!as- serian ganglion and are distributed to the bucco-facial region in flu; branches of the filth cranial nerve.* From the thoracic portion of the sympathetic vasodilator fibers pass to (lie abdominal viscera by way of I Ik: splanchnic nerves and to tin' limbs by way of the branches of the brachial and lumbar plexuses, hut, the data regarding I he dilator fibers lor these regions are QOtasyel entirely satisfactory. Goltz and others have shown that dilator fibers are found in the nerve: of the limbs, hut, the origin of these fibers from the sympa- thetic chain has not, been demon: I rated. * Sec " Recherchee exp6rimentales but le systeme nerveux vasomoteur," D&stre and Mforat, 1884. THE VASOMOTOR NERVES. 597 4. In the nervi ergentes. Eckhard first gave conclusive proof that tha erection of the penis is essentially a vasodilator phenomenon. The fibers arise from the first, second, and third sacral spinal nerves, pass to the hypogastric plexus as the nervi erigentes, and thence are dis- tributed to the erectile tissues of the penis. The General Properties of the Vasodilator Nerve Fibers. — Unlike the vasoconstrictors, the vasodilators are not in tonic activity; at least, no experimental proof has been given that they are. In the case of the erectile tissue of the penis and the dilators of the glands it would seem that the fibers are in activity only during the functional use of the organ, at which time they are excited reflexly. There has been much discussion in physiology as to the nature of the action of the dilator fibers. The muscular coat of the small arteries runs transversely to the length of the vessel, and it is easy to see that when stimulated to greater con- traction through the constrictor fibers it must cause a narrowing of the artery. It is not so evident how the nerve impulses carried by the dilator fibers bring about a widening of the artery. At one time peripheral sympathetic ganglia in the neighborhood of the arteries were used to aid in the explanation, but, since histo- logical evidence of the existence of such ganglia is lacking, the view that seems to meet with most favor at present is as follows: The dilator fibers end presumably in the muscle of the walls of the arteries, and when stimulated their impulses inhibit the tonic contraction of this musculature and thus indirectly bring about a relaxation. Dilatation caused by a vasodilator nerve fiber always presupposes therefore a previous condition of tonic contraction in the walls of the artery, this tonic condition being produced either by the action of vasoconstrictor fibers or possibly by the intrinsic properties of the muscle itself. In the nerves of the limbs, as stated above, both vasoconstrictor and vasodilator effects may be detected by stimulation. It has been shown that the separate fibers may be differentiated by certain differences in properties. Thus, if the peripheral end of the cut sciatic nerve is stimulated by rapidly repeated induction shocks a vasoconstrictor effect is obtained as shown plethysmographically by a diminution in volume of the limb. If, however, the same nerve is stimulated by slowly repeated induction shocks the dilator effect will predominate,* indicating a greater degree of irritability on the part of these latter fibers. After section of the sciatic nerve the vasodilators degen- erate more slowly than the vasoconstrictors, and they retain their irritability when heated or cooled for a longer time than the constrictors. t Vasodilator Center and Vasodilator Reflexes. — Since the * Bowditch and Warren, "Journal of Phvsiologv, " 7, 439, 1886. t Howell, Budgett, and Leonard. "Journal of Physiology," 16, 29S, 1894. 598 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. vasodilator fibers from a system similar to that of the vasocon- strictors, it might be supposed that, like the latter, their activity is controlled from a general center, forming a vasodilator center in the brain similar to the vasoconstrictor center. What evidence we have, however, is against this view. In the dog with his spinal cord severed in the lower thoracic region the penis may show normal erection when the glans is stimulated, — a fact that indicates a reflex center for these dilator fibers in the lumbar cord. For the other clear cases of vasodilator fibers we have no reason at present to believe that they are ail normally connected with a single group of nerve cells located in a definite part of the nervous system. The dilator fibers in the facial, glossopharyngeal, and cervical sympa- thetic (distributed through the trigeminal) all arise probably in the medulla, but not, so far as is known, from a common nucleus. Intimately connected with the question of the existence of a general vasodilator center is the possibility of definite reflex stimulation of the vasodilator fibers. As stated above, reflex dilatation of the blood-vessels may be produced by stimulating various nerve trunks containing afferent fibers. The depressor nerve fibers of the heart give only this effect, and the sensor}- libers from certain other regions, notably the middle ear and the testis, cause mainly, if not exclusively, a fall of arterial pressure due presumably to vascular dilatation. The sensory nerves of the trunk and limbs, when stimulated by the gross methods of the laboratory, give either reflex vasoconstriction or reflex vasodilatation, and, as was stated above, there is reason to believe that these trunks contain two kinds of afferent fibers, — the pressor and the depressor. The action of the former predominates usually, but in deep anesthesia, and particu- larly in those conditions of exposure and exhaustion that precede the appearance of "shock," the depressor effect is most marked or, indeed, may be the only one obtained. To explain such depressor effects we have two possible theories. They may be due to reflex excitation of the centers giving origin to the vasodilator fibers or to reflex inhibition of the tonic activity of the vasoconstrictor centers. The latter explanation is the one usually given, especially for the typical and perhaps special effect of the depressor nerve of the heart. This explanation seems justified by the general consideration that in the two great vascular areas through whose variations in capacity the blood-flow is chiefly regulated,- namely, the abdominal viscera and the skin, — the vasoconstrictor fibers arc chiefly in evidence and are, moreover, in constant tonic activity. On the other hand, the fact that vasodilator fibers exist is presumptive evidence; that they are Stimulated reflexly, since it. is by this means only that- they can normally affect the blood-vessels. Some of the many depressor effects occurring in the body must be due, therefore, to reflex THE VASOMOTOR NERVES. 599 stimulation of the dilators and others to reflex inhibition of the constrictors. It would be convenient to retain the name depressor for the sensory fibers causing the latter effect, and to designate those of the former class by a different name, such as reflex vaso- dilator fibers.* Only experimental work can determine positively to which effect any given reflex dilatation is due, but provisionally at least it would seem justifiable to assume that dilatation by reflex stimulation of the vasodilator fibers occurs in those parts of the body in which vasodilator fibers are known to exist. Thus, the erection of the penis from stimulation of the glans may be explained in this way, also the congestion of the salivary glands during activity, the blushing of the face from emotions, and possibly the dilatation in the skeletal muscles during contraction. Gaskell and others have given reasons for believing that the vessels in the muscles are supplied with vasodilator nerve fibers, and Kleen f has shown that mechanical stimulation of the muscles — kneading, massage, etc. — causes a fall of arterial pressure. Vasodilatation Due to Antidromic Impulses. — The existence of definite effer- ent vasodilator fibers in the nerve trunks to the limbs has been made doubt- ful by the work of Bayliss. This author has discovered certain facts which at present tend to make the question of vasodilatation more obscure, but which, when fully understood, will doubtless give us a much deeper insight into the subject. Briefly stated, he has shownf that stimulation of the posterior roots of the nerves supplying the lumbo-sacral and the brachial plexus causes vas- cular dilatation in the corresponding limbs. He has given reasons for believing that the fibers involved are afferent fibers from the limbs and that, therefore, when stimulated they must conduct the impulses in a direction opposite to the normal — antidromic. It is most difficult to understand how such impulses, conveyed to the terminations of the sensory fibers, can affect the muscular tissue of the blood-vessels. It is most difficult to understand also how such anatomically afferent fibers can be stimulated reflexly in the cen- tral nervous system. Bayliss gives reasons for believing that the limbs receive no vasodilator fibers via the sympathetic system, and that either the blood-vessels in this region are lacking altogether in such fibers or else the afferent fibers function in the way described (see also p. 83). General Schema. — The main facts regarding the vasomotor apparatus may be summarized briefly in tabular form as follows: I. Vasoconstrictor fibers — distributed mainly to the skin and the abdominal viscera (splanch- nic area) , all connected with a general center Efferent vasomotor ) f1 *he m.edulla oblongata, and in constant nerve fibers \ toni(? actlvlty- II. Vasodilator fibers — distributed especially to the erectile tissue, glands, bucco-facial region, and muscles; not connected with a general center and not in tonic activity. * See Hunt, "Journal of Physiology," 18, 381, 1895. f Kleen, " Skandinavisches Archiv f. Physiologie," 247, 1887. t Bayliss, "Journal of Physiology," 26, 173, 1900, and 28, 276, 1902. Afferent fibers giving vasomotor reflexes. 600 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. I. Pressor fibers. Cause vascular constriction and rise of arterial pressure from reflex stimula- tion of the vasoconstrictor center — e. g., sensory nerves of skin. II. Depressor fillers. Cause vascular dilatation and fall of arterial pressure from reflex inhibition of the tonic activity of the vasoconstrictor center, — c. g.. depressor nerve of heart. III. Depressor (or reflex vasodilat or) fibers. Cause vascular dilatation and fall of arterial pres- sure from stimulation of the vasodilator center, — e.g., erectile tissue, congestion of glands in functional activity. It may be supposed that under normal conditions the activity of this mechanism is adjusted so as to control the blood-flow through the different organs in proportion to their needs. When the blood- vessels of a given organ are constricted the flow through that organ is diminished, while that through the rest of the body is increased to a greater or less extent corresponding to the size of the area in- volved in the constriction. When the blood-vessels of a given organ are dilated the blood-flow through that organ is increased and that through the rest of the body diminished more or less. The adaptability of the vascular system is wonderfully complete, and is worked out mainly through the reflex activity of the nervous system exerted partly through the vasomotor fibers and partly through the regulatory nerves of the heart. Regulation of the Blood-supply by Chemical and Mechan- ical Stimuli. — From time to time attention has been called to the fact that the calibre of the blood-vessels may be influenced otherwise than through the agency of vasoconstrictor and vaso- dilator nerve fibers. Gaskell, for example, has shown that acids in slight concent ration cause a vascular dilatation. Bayliss * has recently generalized the facts of this kind, and has suggested that in addition to the nervous regulation described in the preceding pages there may be formed chemical substances of a definite char- acter which exert a similar useful regulating action. As examples of this influence, we have the lactic acid produced in muscles during activity and probably also the carbon dioxid produced in this as in other tissues. These substances may act to produce a local dilatation during functional activity and thus provide the organ with more blood at the time that it is needed. On the other hand, the internal secretioo of the adrenal glands (adrenalin) and possi- bly also of the infundibular portion of the pituitary gland have the reverse effect, causing a vasoconstriction and thus tending to maintain normal vascular tone. In a similar way it ifl probable that the distension of the arteries by internal pres are acts as a me shani- bimulus which lead- to increased tone and thus aid- in main- taining a normal arterial ten-ion. * Baylise in "Ergebnie e der Phy iologie," 1906, v., 319 CHAPTER XXXIII. THE VASOMOTOR SUPPLY OF THE DIFFERENT ORGANS. There are three important organs of the body — namely, the heart, the lungs, and the brain — in which the existence of a vaso- motor supply is still a matter of uncertainty. A very great deal of investigation has been attempted with reference to these organs, but the technical difficulties in each case are so great that no entirely satisfactory conclusion has been reached. A brief review of some of the experimental work on record will suffice to make evident the present condition of our knowledge. Vasomotors of the Heart. — The coronary vessels lie in or on the musculature of the heart. Any variation in the force of con- traction or tonicity of the heart muscle itself will therefore affect possibly the caliber of the arterioles and the rate of blood-flow in the coronary system. At each contraction of the ventricles the coro- nary circulation is probably interrupted by a compression of the smaller arteries and veins, and the size of these vessels during dias- tole will naturally vary with the extent of relaxation of the cardiac muscle. Since stimulation of either of the efferent nerves supplying the heart, vagus and sympathetic, affects the condition of the mus- culature, it is evident at once how difficult it is to distinguish a simultaneous effect upon the coronary arteries, if any such exists. Newell Martin* found that stimulation of the vagus causes dilata- tion of the small arteries on the surface of the heart as seen through a hand lens. Moreover, when the heart is exposed and artificial respiration is stopped the arteries may be seen to dilate before the asphyxia causes any general rise of arterial pressure. Martin interpreted these observations to mean that the coronary arteries receive vasodilator fibers through the vagus. Porterf measured the outflow through the coronary veins in an isolated cat's heart kept alive by feeding it with blood through the coronary arteries. He found that this outflow is diminished when the vagus nerve is stimulated, and hence concluded that the vagus carries vasocon- strictor fibers to the heart. MaasJ reports similar results also obtained from cats' hearts kept alive by an artificial circulation through the coronary arteries. Stimulation of the vagus slowed * Martin, "Transactions Medical and Chirnrgical Faculty of Maryland," 1891. t Porter, "Boston Medical and Surgical Journal," January 9, 1S96. % Maas, " Arehiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 74, 2S1, 1S99. 601 602 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. the stream (vasoconstrictor fibers), while stimulation of the sympathetic path quickened the flow (vasodilator fibers). Neither Maas nor Porter gives conclusive proof that the heart musculature was not affected by the stimulation. Wiggers reports* that the effect of adrenalin upon a heart perfused through the coronary arteries, but not beating, is to decrease the flow, while upon the beating heart this effect is reversed, owing to the action of the adrenalin upon the heart contractions. Schaefer,t on the contrary, gets entirely opposite results. 'When an artificial circulation was maintained through the coronary system and the amount of outflow was determined, he found that this quantity was not definitely influenced by stimulation of either the sympathetic or the vagus branches. Moreover, injection of adrenalin into the coronary circulation had no influence upon the outflow, and since this substance causes an extreme constriction in the vessels of organs provided with vasoconstrictor fibers, the author concludes that the coronary arteries have no vasomotor nerve fibers. It is evident from a consideration of these results that the existence of vasomotor fibers to the heart vessels is still a matter open to investigation. Vasomotors of the Pulmonary Arteries. — The pulmonary circulation is complete in itself and, as was stated on p. 481, it differs from the systemic circulation chiefly in that the peripheral resistance in the capillary area is much smaller. Consequently the arterial pressure in the pulmonary artery is small, while the velocity of the blood-flow is greater than in the systemic circuit, — that is, a larger portion of the energy of the contraction of the right ventricle is used in moving the blood. From the mechanical conditions present it is obvious that the pressure in the pulmonary artery might be increased by a vasoconstriction of the smaller lung arteries, or, on the other hand, by an increase in the blood- flow to the right ventricle through the venae cava?, or, last, bj back pressure from the left auricle when the left ventricle is not emptying itself as well as usual on account of high aortic presstire. While it is comparatively easy, therefore, to measure the pressure in the pulmonary artery, it is difficult, in the interpretation of the changes that occur, to exclude the possibility of the effects being due indirectly to the systemic circulation. Bradford and Dean, J by comparing carefully the simultaneous records of the pressures in flu' aorta and a branch of the pulmonary artery, came to the conclusion that, the latter may be affected independently by stimu- lation of the third, fourth, and fifth thoracic Spinal nerves, and hence concluded that these nerves contain vasoconstrictor fibers * Wiggers, "American Journal of Physiology," 1909. Proceedings of the American Physiological Society. | "Archive de cience biologiques, " LI, suppl. volume, 251, L905. X Bradford and Dean, " Journal of Physiology, " 16,34, L894. VASOMOTOR SUPPLY OF THE ORGANS. 603 to the pulmonary vessels, the course of the fibers being, in general, that taken by the accelerator fibers to the heart, namely, to the first thoracic sympathetic ganglion by the rami communicantes and thence to the pulmonary plexus. They give evidence to show- that these fibers are stimulated during asphyxia. The authors state, however, that the effects obtained upon the pressure in the pulmonary artery are relatively and absolutely small as compared with the vasomotor effects in the aortic system. Similar results have been obtained by other observers (Francois-Franck). Using another and more direct method, Brodie and Dixon* have come to an opposite conclusion. These authors maintained an artificial circulation through the lungs and measured the rate of outflow when the nerves supplying the lungs were stimulated. Under these conditions stimulation of the vagus or the sympathetic caused no definite change in the rate of flow, — a result which would indicate that neither nerve conveys vasomotor fibers to the lung vessels. This conclusion was strengthened by the fact that in similar per- fusions made upon other organs (intestines) vasomotor effects were easily demonstrated. Moreover, adrenalin, pilocarpin, and mus- carin cause marked vasoconstriction when irrigated through the intestine, but have no such effect upon the vessels in the lungs. These authors conclude that the lung vessels have no vasomotor nerves at all, and their experimental evidence might be accepted as satisfactory except for the fact that a similar method in the hands of another observer has given opposite results. Plumiert finds that the outflow through a perfused lung is diminished in some cases by stimulation of the sympathetic branches to the lungs, and also by the use of adrenalin. Under such conditions it is necessary to defer a decision until more experiments are reported. Regarding the vasomotors of the lungs, one can only say, as in the case of the heart, that their existence has not been demonstrated. The Circulation in the Brain and Its Regulation. — The question of the existence of vasomotor nerves to the brain brings up necessarily the larger question of the special characteristics of the cranial circulation. The brain is contained in a rigid box so that its free expansion or contraction with variations in the amount of blood can not take place as in other organs and we have to con- sider in how far this fact modifies its circulation. The Arterial Supply of the Brain. — The brain is supplied through the two internal carotids and the two vertebrals, which together form the circle of Willis. It will be remembered also that the vertebral arteries give off the posterior and the anterior spinal arteries, which supply the spinal cord, and that the last-named artery makes anastomoses along the cord with the intercostal arteries * Brodie and Dixon, "Journal of Physiology," 30, 476, 1904. f Plumier, "Journal de physiologie et de pathologie generate," 6, 665, 1904; see also "Archives internationales de physiologie," 1, 189, 1904. 604 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. and other branches from the descending aorta. From the ana- tomical arrangement alone it is evident that the circulation in the brain is very well protected from the possibility of being inter- rupted by the accidental closure of one or more of its arteries. In some animals, the dog, one can ligate both internal carotids and both vertebrals without causing unconsciousness or the death of the animal. In an animal under these conditions a collateral circula- tion must be brought into play through the anastomoses of the spinal arteries. In man, on the contrary, it is stated that ligation of both carotids is dangerous or fatal. The Venous Supply. — The venous system of the brain is peculiar, especially in the matter of the venous sinuses. These large spaces are contained between folds of the dura mater or, on the base of the skull, between the dura mater and the bone. The channel hollowed out in the bone is covered with a roof of tough, inex- tensible dura mater, and indeed in some animals the basal sinuses SKulL <$ui arachnoid/' f <5payce. ' Piaftlatir. CerehruftL Fig. 248. — Diagram to represent the relations of the meningeal membranes of the cere- brum, the position of the subarachnoidal space and of the venous sinuses. may in part be entirely incased in bone. The larger cerebral veins open into these sinuses; the openings have no valves, but, on the contrary, are kept patent and protected from closure by the struc- ture of the dura mater around the orifice. The smaller veins are very thin walled and free from valves. The venous blood emerges from the skull in man mainly through the opening of the lateral .sinuses into the internal jugular vein, although there is also a communication in the orbit between the cavernous sinus and the ophthalmic veins through which the cranial blood may pass into the system of facial veins, another communication with the venous plexuses of the cord, and a Milliliter of small emissary veins. In some of (he lower animals -the dog, for instance — the main outflow is into the externa] jugular through what is known as the superior cerebral vein. A point of physiological interest is that the venous sinuses and their points of emergence from the skull are by their structure well protected from closure by compression. VASOMOTOR SUPPLY OF THE ORGANS. 605 The Meningeal Spaces. — The general arrangement of the menin- geal membranes, and particularly of the meningeal spaces, is im- portant in connection with the mechanics of the brain circulation. In the skull the dura mater adheres to the bone, the pia mater invests closely the surface of the brain, while between lies the arachnoid (Fig. 248). The capillary space between the arachnoid and the dura, the so-called subdural space, may be neglected. Between the arachnoid and the pia mater, however, lies the sub- arachnoidal space more or less intersected by septa of connective tissue, but in free communication throughout the brain and cord. This subarachnoidal space is filled with a liquid, the cerebro- spinal liquid, which forms a pad inclosing the brain and cord on all sides. The liquid surrounding the cord is in free communication with that in the brain, as is indi- cated in the accompanying schematic figure (Fig. 249). Within the brain itself there are certain points at the an- gles and hollows of the differ- ent parts of the brain at which the subarachnoidal space is much enlarged, forming the so-called cisternal, which are in communication one with another by means of the less conspicuous canals (see Fig. 250) . The whole system is also in direct communica- tion with the ventricles of the brain on the one hand, through the foramen of Magendie, the foramina of Luschka, and perhaps at other places, and on the other hand, along the cranial and spinal nerves it is continued outward in the tissue spaces of the sheaths of these nerves. The Pacchionian bodies constitute also a peculiar feature of the sub- arachnoidal space. These bodies occur in numbers that vary with the individual and with age, and are found along the sinuses, especially the superior longitudinal sinus. Each body is a minute, .Break.. Cerebrv-Jfeutal Liau-ui- jj/nnai Columrv. .Dura Mater. Cerebri) Spinal liautd. jJhinal Cord, Fig. 249. — Diagram to show the connec- tion of the subarachnoidal space in the brain and the cord. 606 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. pear-shaped protrusion of the arachnoidal membrane into the inte- rior of a sinus, as represented schematically in Fig. 251. Through these bodies the cerebrospinal liquid is brought into close contact with the venous blood, the two being sepa- rated only by a thin layer of dura and the very thin arachnoid. The number of the Pacchionian bodies is hardly sufficient to lead us to suppose that they have a special physiological impor- tance. The cerebrospinal liq- uid found in the subarach- noidal space and the ventri- cles of the brain is a very thin, watery liquid having a specific gravity of only 1.007 to 1.008. It contains only traces of proteins and other organic substances, which may vary under pathological condi- tions. It is thinner and more watery than the lymph, resembling rather the aqueous humor of the eye. The amount of this fluid Fig. 2nii — Diagram to show the location of the cisternse and canals of the subarach- noidal space. — (Poirier and Charpy.) I i;-. 251 Schema to haw the relations <>f the Pacchionian bodies to the sinuses: d, d, I "i'i "i the dura mater, inclosing a sinus between them J ■>>.'<., the blood in the .■inn ; a, the arachnoidal membrane; i>. the pia mater; I'"-, the Pacchionian body as a projection "i the arachnoid into the blood sinu , present normally is difficult bo determine. Various figures have been given, but il is usually stated to amount to 60 t<> 80 <•.<•. If VASOMOTOR SUPPLY OF THE ORGANS. 607 these figures are correct it evidently does not form a thick envelope to the nervous system. Under abnormal conditions (hydroceph- alus, etc.) the quantity may be greatly increased. It is physio- logically interesting to find that this liquid may be formed very promptly from the blood and, when in excess, be absorbed quickly by the blood. In fractures of the base of the skull, for instance, the liquid has been observed to drain off steadily at the rate of 200 c.c. or more per day. On the other hand, when one injects physio- logical saline into the subarachnoidal space under some pressure it is absorbed with surprising rapidity. After death, also, the liquid present in the subarachnoidal space is soon absorbed. Intracranial Pressure. — By intracranial pressure is meant the pressure in the space between the skull and the brain, — therefore the pressure in the subarachnoidal liquid and presumably also the pressure in the ventricles of the brain, since the two spaces are in communication. This pressure may be measured by boring a hole through the skull, dividing the dura, and connecting the under- lying space with a manometer. Observers who have measured this pressure state that it is always the same as the venous pressure within the sinuses. This we can understand when we remember the close relations between the subarachnoidal liquid and the large veins and sinuses. We may consider that the large veins are sur- rounded by the cerebrospinal liquid, and consequently an equilib- rium of pressure must be established between them ; any rise in the intracranial pressure raises venous pressure by compression of the veins. This statement holds true at least so far as the intracranial pressure is due to the circulation. Variations of pressure from pathological causes — tumors, clots, abscesses, etc. — may exercise apparently a local effect. The intracranial pressure is caused and controlled normally by the pressure within the arteries and capillaries. This pressure, by enlarging these vessels, tends to expand the brain against the skull, and exercises a pressure, there- fore, upon the intervening cerebrospinal liquid. This pressure, however, can not exceed that in the veins, since, as said, an ex- cess will be equalized by a corresponding compression of the veins. The venous pressure in the end determines, therefore, the actual amount of intracranial pressure. Conditions which alter the pressure in the cerebral veins affect the intracranial pressure correspondingly. Thus, compression of the veins of the neck raises the pressure in the cerebral veins and also intracranial pressure, and a higher general arterial pressure also results finally in a higher pressure in the cerebral veins and therefore in the subarachnoidal space. Reduced to its simplest form, the conditions may be represented by a schema such as is given in Fig. 252. A system with an artery, capillary area, 608 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. . d V t/ and a vein is represented as inclosed in a rigid box and surrounded by an incompressible liquid. According to tbe conditions prevailing in the body, the pressure in the interior of .4. and its branches is much higher than in V. If, now, the pressure in A is increased the greater pressure brought to bear on the walls will tend to expand them; a greater pressure will thereby be communicated to the outside liquid, which, in turn, will compress the veins correspondingly. The expansion on the arterial side is made possible by a corresponding diminution on the venous side where the internal pressure is least. The recorded measure- ments of the intracranial pressure show that it may vary from 50 to 60 mms. of mercury, obtained during the great rise of pressure following strychnin poisoning, to zero or less, as obtained by Hill * from a man while in the erect pos- ture. In this position the negative influence of gravity is at its maximum. The Effect of Variations in Arterial Pressure upon the Blood-flow through the Brain. — Quite a number of observers f have proved ex- perimentally that a rise of general arterial pressure is fol- lowed, not only by an increase in the intracranial tension, but also by an increased blood-flow through the brain. There has been much discussion as to whether a rise of arterial pressure in the basilar arteries can cause any actual increase in the amount of blood in the brain or whether it expresses if self solely or mainly as an increased amount of flow. In the other organs of the body, except perhaps the bones, a general lie of pressure, not accompanied by a constriction of the organ's own arteries, causes a dilatation or congestion of the organ together with an increased blood-flow. Physiologically the congestion — that is, 1 be' increased capacity of the vessels — is of no value; the important tiling is the increase in the quantity of blood flowing * Bayliss and Hill, "Journal of Physiology," is, 356, 1895 t See Gartner and Wagner, "Wiener med. Wochenschrift," 1SS7; de Boeck and Verhorgen, "Journal de M6decine, etc.," Brussels; Roy and Sher- rington, "Journal of Physiology," ll, 85, 1890; Reiner and Schnitzler, "Archiv f. exp. Pathol, u. Pharmakol.," 38, 249, 1897. Fig. 252. — Schema to represent the transmission of arterial pressure through the brain substance to the veins: A, The artery, V, the vein, represented as entering into and emerging from a box with rigid walls and filled with incompressible liquid ; c, c, the intervening area of small arte- ries, etc. An expansion of the walls of the arterial system by the pulse wave or by a rise of arterial pressure increases the pres- sure on the surrounding liquid and this is transmitted through the liquid to the walls of the veins and compresses them, since at this point of the circuit the intravascular pre ure is low. VASOMOTOR SUPPLY OF THE ORGANS. 609 through. In the brain, owing to the peculiarities of its position, it has been suggested that perhaps no actual increase in size is possible. It is evident, however, that the existence of the liquid in the subarachnoidal space makes possible some actual expan- sion of the organ. For as the pressure upon this liquid increases it may be driven into the dural sac of the cord (Fig. 249) and along the sheaths of the cranial and spinal nerves. To what extent this is actually possible in man we do not know, nor do we know how much cerebrospinal liquid is contained in the skull and brain of man. In the dog Hill* finds experimentally that the brain can expand only by an amount equal to 2 or 3 c.c. without causing a rise of intra- cranial tension ; so that probably these figures represent the amount of expansion possible in this animal by simple squeezing out of the cerebrospinal liquid. If the rise of arterial pressure is such as to expand the brain beyond this point, then it may not only force out cerebrospinal liquid, if any remains, but, as explained in the last paragraph, it will compress the veins and raise intracranial Fig. 253. — Simultaneous record of puise in the circle of Willis (c) and in the torcu- lar Herophili (t). The tracing from the circle of Willis was obtained by means of a Hiirthle manometer connected with the head end of the internal carotid. It will be noted that the pulses are simultaneous, indicating that the venous pulse is due to the transmis- sion of the arterial pulse through the brain substance. pressure. To the extent that the veins are compressed as the ar- teries expand no actual increase in the size or blood-capacity of the brain takes place. That an expansion of the brain arteries com- presses the veins is indicated very clearly by the normal occurrence of a venous pulse in this organ. The blood flows out of the veins of the brain in pulses synchronous with the arterial pulses, and this venous pulse may be recorded easily as shown in Fig. 253. In this case the sudden expansion of the arteries compresses the cerebral veins, giving a synchronous rise of pressure in the interior of the sinuses. Some authors (Geigel, Grashey), on purely theoretical grounds, have held that this compression of the veins may result in a diminished blood-flow through the organ, — a sort of self-strangu- lation of its own circulation. Actual experiment shows that this is not the case. Any ordinary rise of general arterial pressure is accompanied by a greater blood-flow through the brain, and * Hill, "The Physiology and Pathology of the Cerebral Circulation." London, 1896. 39 610 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. the author* has shown that sudden variations of arterial pressure far beyond possible normal limits cause no blocking of the venous outflow. Whether the brain increases in volume as a result of a rise of arterial pressure is, on the physiological side, unimportant; the main point is that the amount of blood flowing through it is increased under such circumstances as would cause a like result in other organs. That the compression of the veins does not produce any sensible obstruction to the blood-flow may be under- stood easily. In the first place, this compression does not take place at the narrow exit from the skull, — since at that point the sinuses are protected from the action of intracranial pressure. The compression takes place doubtless upon the cerebral veins emptying into the sinuses, and at this point the venous bed, taken as a whole, is so large that the expansion due to an ordinary rise of arterial pressure is distributed and has but little effect on the volume of the flow. Secondly, very great increases in arterial pressure, up to the point of rupture of the walls, have less and less effect in actually expanding the arteries; a point is reached eventually at which these tubes become practically rigid, so that farther expansion is- impossible. This, of course, is true for every organ. The Regulation of the Brain Circulation. — It is still a matter of uncertainty whether the arteries of the brain possess vasomotor nerves. Most of the authors who have studied the matter experi- mentally have concluded that there are none.f These authors were unable to show that stimulation of any of the nerve paths that might innervate the brain vessels causes local effects upon the brain circulation. Whenever such stimulations caused a change in pres- sure or amount of flow in the brain the result was referable to an alter- ation of general arterial pressure produced by a vasomotor change elsewhere in the body. When as a result of such stimulation the pressure rises in the circle of Willis, one may infer' that if this is due to a local constriction in the cerebral arterioles there should be a fall of pressure in the venous sinuses and a diminished flow of blood; if, on the contrary, it is due to a constriction elsewhere in the body that has increased general arterial pressure, but has not constricted the brain circuit, then I here should he a rise in venous pressure and intracranial pressure, together with a greater flow of blood through the brain. .Most, observers obtain this latter result. Some inves- tigators, Hiirthle, Franeois-Franck, and others,! on the other * Howell, "American Journal of Physiology. " I, 57, L898. t See Roy and Sherrington, Bayliss and Hill, Hill, Gaertner and Wagner, inc. rii ., and 1 1 ill and MacLeod, "Journal of Physiology, " 2<>, 394, 1901. % Httrthle, "Archiv f. diegesammte Physiologie, 4 1, 674, L889; Franeois- Franck, "Archives de physiol. normale h pathologique, " L890; Cavazanni, "Archives itah'ennes de biologie," l'.», 214, 1898; M filler and Siebeck, "Zeit- Bchrifl r. exp. Path. a. Therapie," 4, 57, 1907. VASOMOTOR SUPPLY OF THE ORGANS. 611 hand, have obtained results, especially from stimulation of the cervical sympathetic, which indicated local vasoconstriction or vaso- dilatation in the brain. It would seem, however, that these latter observers have not excluded the possibility that the variations in pressure obtained by them were due to reflex effects upon the blood-vessels of the body, especially as Frangois-Franck has shown that the sympathetic in the neck contains afferent fibers which give such reflexes.* In experiments made upon an isolated brain (in the skull) perfused with an artificial circulation, Wiggers f states that addition of adrenalin caused a diminution in the outflow from the organ, thus showing that the adrenalin had caused a constriction somewhere in the circuit. If, as some authors believe, adrenalin acts only on plain muscle that is innervated by sj^mpathetic nerve fibers this result furnishes indirect evidence for the existence of such fibers in the case of the brain vessels. Using the same method this author states that electrical stimu- lation applied directly to the sheath of the internal carotid at its entrance into the skull also causes a decrease in the outflow, a fact which would indicate the existence of constrictor fibers running in the sheath of this artery. As another argument in favor of the presence of vasomotor fibers, it may be men- tioned that a number of observers — Gulland, Huber, Hunter J — have demonstrated that the vessels of the brain are provided with perivascular nerve plexuses. It must be admitted, how- ever, that this histological fact is not satisfactory unless it is supplemented by experimental evidence. Judged from this latter standpoint, we have no convincing proof at present of the existence of cerebral vasomotors, although it must be granted that recent evidence rather tends in that direction. If vaosmotors are present it is possible that they may serve to control the distribution of blood within the cerebral area, while the general supply to the brain as a whole is increased or decreased by a mechanism of another sort described by Roy and Sherring- ton. According to these authors the blood-flow through the brain is controlled indirectly by vasomotor effects upon the rest of the body. When, for example, a vasoconstriction occurs in the skin or the splanchnic area the result is a rise of pressure in the aorta and, therefore, a rise of pressure in the circle of Willis, which then forces more blood through the brain. Adopting this view, we can understand the teleology of certain well-known vaso- motor reflexes. Stimulation of the skin generally causes a reflex * Frangois-Franck, "Journal de phys. et de path, gen.," 1, 724, 1889. t Wiggers, '"American Journal of Physiology," 1905, 14, 452; and 21, 454, 1908. t See Hunter, "Journal of Physiologie, " 26, 465, 1902. 612 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. constriction and rise of pressure, and one can well understand that this result is valuable if it means a greater flow of blood through the brain, since under the conditions of nature such stimulation, especially when painful, demands alertness and increased activity on the part of the animal. Attention has also been called to the fact that in plethysmography observations on man the most certain and extensive constrictions of the skin vessels are those caused by increased mental activity. Mosso has shown by observa- tions upon men with trephine holes in the skull that the constriction of the limbs is always accompanied by a dilatation of the brain. This fact, therefore, fits exactly the view that is being considered. The peripheral constriction, by raising general blood-pressure, dilates the brain more or less, and, what is more important, drives more blood through it. It is difficult to understand why psychical activity is always associated in this way with a peripheral con- striction unless the object of the reflex is to increase the blood- supply to the brain. Even if vasomotor fibers are subsequently shown to be present in the brain, the importance of this reflex in providing a greater flow to the central organ at the time that it is in activity may still be admitted. A general irrigation, so to speak, is provided for by this means. Local vasomotors may be used to divert this flow mainly through one or another cerebral area. Vasomotor Nerves of the Head Region. — The vasomotor supply of the various parts of the head, including the mouth cavity, has been investigated by many observers. It would appear from the results of most of these investigations that the vasoconstrictor supply for the skin, including the ears, the eye, the mouth, and buccal glands, is derived mainly, if not entirely, from the sympa- thetic nervous system. These fibers arise from tho spinal cord in the upper thoracic nerves, first to the fifth or sixth, emerge by the rami communicantes to the sympathetic chain, in which they pass upward and end, for the most part, in the superior cervical ganglion. From this ganglion they are distributed, by various routes, as postgan- glionic fibers. In one interesting instance the constrictor fibers for the head were supposed to take a somewhat different course. it was shown by Schiff, long ago, thai in the rabbil the ear receives vasomotor fibers from the auricularis magnus nerve, a branch of the third cervical nerve. Later investigations indicate (Meltzer) that the ear, in fact, receives most of its vasoconstrictor fibers by this route. Fletcher, however, has shown that these fibers do not emerge from the brain in the roots of the third cervical, but rather in the general outflow from the thoracic region. After reaching the sym- pathetic chain these particular U\>rr* pass to the third cervical by the gray rami from the first thoracic ganglion, which communicate with a number of the cervical nerves. On the other hand, the VASOMOTOR SUPPLY OF THE ORGANS. 613 vasodilator fibers for the head are supplied in part by way of the cervical sympathetic, following the same general path as the con- strictors, and in part by way of the cranial nerves (seventh, ninth) and the sympathetic ganglia with which they connect. According to Langley, the outflow of the seventh nerve passes to the spheno- palatine ganglion, whence as postganglionic fibers they accompany the branches of the superior maxillary nerve and cause vasodila- tation in the membrane of the nose, soft palate, tonsils, uvula, roof of mouth, upper lips, gums, and pharynx. The well-known dilators of the submaxillary and sublingual glands are contained in the chorda tympani branch of the seventh nerve; the preganglionic fibers terminate probably in the small peripheral ganglia connected with these glands. The fibers that emerge in the ninth pass in part directly to the tongue and in part terminate first in the otic ganglion, whence they are distributed with the branches of the inferior maxillary to the lower lips, cheeks, gums, and parotid and orbital glands. Dastre and Morat describe the vasodilators in the cervical sympathetic as reaching the fifth cranial nerve by com- municating branches from the superior cervical ganglion and state that they cause dilatation of the bucco-facial region, — that is, the lips, the gums, cheeks, palate, nasal mucous membrane, and the corresponding skin areas. The Trunk and the Limbs. — The vasoconstrictor fibers for these regions are distributed, so far as is known, chiefly to the skin. They are all derived immediately from the sympathetic chain and ultimately from the outflow in the anterior roots of the thoracic and lumbar spinal nerves. Those for the upper limbs arise from the midthoracic region chiefly (fourth to ninth thoracic nerves), those for the lower limbs arise in the nerves of the lower thoracic and upper lumbar region (eleventh, twelfth, thirteenth thoracic [dog] and first and second lumbar). The vasodilator fibers in the nerves of the limbs have been demonstrated frequently, as already explained. Whether or not these fibers also pass through the sympathetic system, following the same general course as the vaso constrictors, has not been shown conclusively. The most definite work at present (Bayliss) indicates that the vasodilator effect is directly caused in some unknown way by fibers found in the posterior roots of the nerves forming the brachial and the sciatic plexus. The unsatisfactory explanations offered for this result have been referred to (p. 599). The Abdominal Organs. — The stomach and intestines receive their most important supply of vasoconstrictor fibers by way of the splanchnic nerves and celiac ganglion. These fibers emerge from the cord in the lower thoracic spinal nerves, from the fifth down, and the upper lumbar nerves, and they supply the whole mesenteric 61-4 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. circulation as far as the descending colon. According to some observers (Francois-Franck and Hallion), the mesenteric vessels receive a supply of vasodilator fibers by the same general route, and it is also stated that similar fibers reach this region through the vagus nerve. Concerning this latter statement at least further con- firmation is necessary. The pancreas has been shown to receive vasoconstrictor fibers by way of the splanchnics. and the kidney, according to Bradford, receives vasodilator as well as vasocon- stricter fibers from the same nerve. Most of the vasomotor fibers to the kidney of the dog emerge from the cord in the roots of the eleventh, twelfth, and thirteenth thoracic nerves, and those for the liver (Francois-Franck and Hallion) come from about the same region. The vasoconstrictors to the spleen are said to leave the spinal cord chiefly in the anterior roots of the sixth, seventh, and eighth thoracic nerves. The Genital Organs. — Both vasoconstrictor and vasodilator fibers have been discovered for the external genital organs (penis, scrotum, clitoris, vulva). . The vasoconstrictors arise in the dog from the thirteenth thoracic to the fourth lumbar nerves, pass over to the sympathetic chain, and thence reach the organs either by way of the hypogastric nerve and pelvic plexus or by way of the sacral sympathetic ganglia and their branches to the pudic nerves. The vasodilator fibers arise from the sacral spinal nerve, being the best known of the sacral autonomic system. They enter the ner- vus erigens and thence reach the organs by way of the pelvic plexus. The especial importance of these fibers in the process of erection is described in the section on the physiology of the repro- ductive organs. The internal genital organs — uterus, vagina, leferens, seminal vesicles, etc. — receive no vasomotor fibers from the sacral autonomic system, — that is, from the nervi erigentes —but do receive a supply of constrictor fibers from the sympathetic system. These latter fibers emerge from the cord in the roots of the upper lumbar nerves and reach the organs by way of the in- ferior mesenteric ganglion and hypogastric nerve.* Vasomotor Supply of the Skeletal Muscles.— Gaskellf es- peeially has given evidence of the existence of vasomotor libers in t In- muscles. I [e concludes, as the result of his work, thai I he blood- vessels of the muscles receive both vasoconstrictor and vasodilator liber.-, but thai the latter greatly predominate, at least, their physiological effeel Is much more evident iii experimental work. As proof of the pr< ence of dilator fibers he gives such results as these: The mylohyoid muscle of the frog is thin enough to be • For the bibliography of the vasomotor apply to the various organs see Langley, "Ergebni e aer Physiologie," vol. ii., pari a., p. 820, 1903. keil, Journal of Physiology," 1,262, L878 7'.). VASOMOTOR SUPPLY OF THE ORGANS. 615 observed directly under the microscope. When curarized and stimulated through its motor nerve the small vessels may be seen to dilate and there is an augmented flow of blood. In a dog section of the motor nerve to a muscle is followed by a greatly increased flow of blood, which, however, is only temporary and is referable to a mechanical stimulation of the dilator fibers. Direct stimulation of the severed nerve causes an increased flow of blood through the muscles, but if the muscles are first completely curarized stimulation causes, on the contrary, a decreased flow. This last result is ex- plained on the supposition that curare paralyzes the endings of the dilator fibers and thus allows the effects of the constrictors to mani- fest themselves. Since, however, Bayliss has given evidence to show (p. 599) that the dilator effect in the limbs is due to the anti- dromic action of afferent fibers, it is evident that this important question needs reinvestigation. Various physiologists have shown that muscular activity is accompanied by an increase in the blood- flow through the muscle, as we should expect, but it remains uncer- tain whether this result is brought about solely by an increased activity of the heart or by the combined effect of vasodilatation and increase in heart-work. Kaufmann * takes this latter view in con- sequence of some interesting results obtained upon horses. He measured the blood-flow through the masseter muscle and the elevator of the lip in a horse in which the muscles were exercised normally by the act of eating. The blood-flow was increased as much as five times over that observed during rest, and that this increase was due in part at least to a local dilatation seems to be proved by the fact that the blood-pressure in the artery supplying the muscle fell, while that in the vein rose. While, therefore, our experimental knowledge of the vasomotors of the muscles needs further investigation, we may provisionally accept the view ad- vocated by Gaskell, — namely, that the vasomotor supply to the muscles consists essentially of dilator fibers and that these fibers are brought into action reflexly whenever the muscles contract, thus providing an increased blood-flow in proportion to the func- tional activity. It should be added that the local dilatation in the muscles during activity may be due also to the chemical action of the (acid) metabolic products on the blood-vessels (p. 600). The Vasomotor Nerves to the Veins. — It is assumed in physi- ology that the vasoconstrictors and vasodilators end in the muscula- ture of the small arteries. The veins also have a muscular coat, and it is possible that if this musculature were innervated from the central nervous system we should have another efficient factor in controlling the blood-flow. Mall has given very clear proof that * Kaufmann, "Archives de physiologie normale et pathologique," 1892, pp. 279 and 495. 616 CIRCULATION OF BLOOD AND LYMPH. the portal vein receives vasoconstrictor fibers from the splanchnic nerve.* but this supply may be exceptional, as the portal system itself is unique. The portal vein, indeed, plays the role physiolog- ically of an artery in regard to the liver. Roy and Sherrington f give some evidence for the existence of venomotor nerves to the large veins of the neck, and Thompson, as also Bancroft, J reports experiments in which it was found that stimulation of the sciatic nerve caused a visible constriction of the superficial veins of the hind limbs. The whole subject, however, of venomotor nerves has been but little investigated, and at present little or no use is made of this possible system in explaining the facts of the circulation, although it is very evident that if such a system exists, controlling the tonicity of the veins, it must exert a very important influence in regulating the supply of blood to the heart . § THE CIRCULATION OF THE LYMPH. The direction of flow of the lymph is from the tissues toward the large lymphatic trunks, the thoracic and the right lymphatic duct. The flow is maintained in this direction mainly by a difference in pressure at the two ends. At the opening of the large trunks into the jugular veins the pressure is very- low; in the vein, in fact, it may be zero or even negative as compared with the atmospheric pressure. The opening between the lymph vessel and the vein is protected by a valve which opens toward the vein, and the lymph, therefore, will flow into the vein as long as the pressure in the latter is lower than that in the lymphatic- duct. At the other extremity of the system, in the tissue spaces to which the lymphatic capillaries are distributed, the pressure, on the contrary, is high. Its exact amount is not known. But, since the pressure in the blood capillaries is equal to 40 60 nims. Hg, the pressure in the liquid of the surrounding tissues must also be consider- able. The tissues are, in fact, in a condition of turgidity owing to the pressure of the lymph in the tissue-spaces. This difference in pressure at the two ends of the lymphatic system is the main constant factor in mov- ing the lymph. It is obvious that in the long run it is dependent, upon the pressure within the blood-vessels and therefore upon the force of the heart Deal . The cont ract ion- of t Ik; heart supply t he energy, not only for the move- men i of the blood, bul also for the much slower movement of t he lymph. The circulation of the lymph is aided, however, by many accessory factors. In Some animal- t here are genuine lymph hearts upon t he course of t he vessels, — that i-. pulsatile expansions of the lymph vessels whose force of heat., con- trol led by valves, i- aired ly applied to moving the lymph. No such struct ures an- found in the mammalia, bul according to some observers the large re- ceptacle at the beginning of the thoracic duct, receptaculum chyli may undergo contractions, and is, besides, under the influence of motor and inhibitory nerves. Such movements, if they occur, must be equivalent to the action of a lymph heart in their influence upon the How of lymph. The (low of lymph or chyle in the intestinal area is also, without, doubt, greatly I d by I he peri -la It ic and especially by I he rhyl Imiic cont ract. ions of the musculature ol the intestines. The volume of the lymph in this region is especially large and the lymph capillaries and veins are provided with valves. Rhythmical contractions of the musculature of the intestine must squeeze * \l.dl, "Archiv f. Physiologic," p. 109, 1892. Loy and Sherrington, "Journal of Physiology," II, 85, 1890. 1 Bancroft, " American Journal of Physiology," I, -177, 1898. 3ee I lenderson, " American Journal of Physiology," 23, '■> 15, 1909. VASOMOTOR SUPPLY OF THE ORGANS. 617 the lymph toward the thoracic duct, acting like a local pump to accelerate the flow of lymph. A similar influence is exerted by the contractions of the skeletal muscles. The compression exerted by the shortened fibers squeezes the lymph vessels and, on account of the valves present, forces the lymph onward toward the larger ducts. The flow of lymph from the resting muscles — the arms and legs, for instance — is normally small in quantity, but during muscular exercise and massage it is obviously increased. This increase may be observed in experimental work by placing a cannula in the thoracic duct. Active or passive movements of the limbs under these conditions will cause a noticeable increase in the outflow from the duct. Still another factor which exercises an influence upon the flow of lymph is found in the respiratory move- ments of the thorax. At each inspiration the pressure within the thorax is diminished (increase of negative pressure), and this factor influences the lymph flow in several ways : By increasing the flow of blood through the large veins at the edge of the thorax, jugulars and subclavians, it doubtless aspirates lymph from the thoracic and right lymphatic ducts into these veins. More- over, by lowering the pressure upon the intrathoracic portion of the thoracic duct it also aspirates the lymph from the abdominal portion of this vessel. When we place a cannula in the thoracic duct and measure the outflow directly it is found to be exceedingly slow and variable. Older measure- ments (Weiss) indicate that it has a velocity in the duct in the neck of about 4 mms. _ per second, but this velocity changes naturally with the conditions influencing the production of lymph in the tissues. Heidenhain estimates that for a dog weighing 10 kgms. the total outflow from the thoracic duct in 24 hours is equal to 640 c.c. Munk and Rosenstein, from observations upon a case with a lymph fistula, estimated that in man the flow may be equal to 50 to 100 or 120 c.c. per hour. SECTION VI. PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. Historical. — The term respiration as usually employed in physiology refers to the process of gaseous exchange between an organism and its environment. This exchange consists essentially in the absorption of oxygen b}r the living matter and the elimination of carbon dioxid. It is one of the generalizations of physiology that all living matter, with the exception perhaps of the anaerobic organisms, requires oxygen for its vital processes — that is, for the development of its energy requirements. On the other hand, one of the universal end-products of this metabolism is carbon dioxid. Hence, respiration in some form is one great characteristic of living things. In the simplest animals and plants, the unicellular organisms, the exchange between the air (or water) and the organism takes place directly, but in the more complex animals some form of respiratory appara- tus is developed whose function consists either in bringing the air or oxygen-laden water to the constituent cells, as in the air tubes of the insects, or in bringing the circulating blood into contact with the air or water, as in the case of animals provided with lungs or gills. In man and the air-breathing vertebrates the latter device is employed and one may distinguish in such animals between internal and external respiration. By the latter term is meant the gaseous exchange, absorption of oxygen and elimination of carbon dioxid, that takes place in the lungs between the blood in the pul- monary capillaries and the air in the alveoli. By internal respira- tion is meanl the similar exchange that takes place in the systemic capillaries between the blood and the tissue elements. All of this exchange is, so to speak, secondary, since the essential process consists in the history of the oxygen after it is absorbed into the tissues, thai is, the part taken by theoxygen in the metabolism of living matter. This process, however, is a part of the subject of nutrition. The food absorbed from the digestive organs and the oxygen taken from the blood have a common history, or at least their reactions are indissolubly connected after they come within the field Of influence of the living molecules. This side of the func- tion of the oxygen may he considered, therefore, more appropriately in the section on nutrition. In the present section attention will he directed to the heautiful means that have been adapted to the pur- of supplying the tissues with oxygen and of removing the carbon dioxid. 618 HISTORICAL. 619 The true understanding of the object of the act of respiration we owe to Lavoisier, the discoverer of oxygen. In his paper published in 1777, entitled "Experiments on the Respiration of Animals and on the Changes which the Air Undergoes in Passing through the Lungs," he laid the foundations of our present knowledge, and in subsequent work he developed a conception of the nature of physiological oxidations which has dominated the physiological theories of nutrition up to the present time. The discovery of the physiological meaning of respiration and the function of the lungs constitutes the most interesting part of the history of physiology. All the great physiologists of past ages contributed their part to the story, and as we look back we can count distinctly the different steps made toward the truth as we understand it to-day. The history of this subject is not only most instructive in demonstrating the triumphant although slow progress of scientific investigation, but it illustrates well also the intimate interrelations of physiology with the sister sciences of chemistry and physics and the great value of the experimental method. The theory of respiration held in each century was formulated to explain, as far as possible, the facts that were known, and as we look back from our vantage point it is most impressive to realize how well-known phenomena, imperfectly understood, were apparently explained by theories which we now know to be incorrect. Without doubt, many of the explanations accepted to-day will in later times be found to rest upon a similar incomplete knowledge. Each generation must do the best it can with the knowledge of its times. The history of respiration, the successive steps in its progress may be summarized in a few words. Aristotle thought that the main function of respiration is to regulate the heat of the body, which was supposed to be produced in the heart; hence the increased respira- tions after muscular exercise when the body-heat is increased. At the same time he believed, with the philosophers of his times, that the body receives something from the air that is necessary to life, a subtle something that he designated as the " pneuma." Praxagoras taught that blood is contained only in the veins, and that the ar- teries are filled with a gaseous substance, the "pneuma" derived from the air, an unfortunate error that prevailed in medicine for several centuries. The two celebrated anatomists and physiologists of the Alexandrian school, Herophilus and Erasistratus, distin- guished two kinds of pneuma, the vital spirits, which are made or extracted from the air in the lungs and whose production consti- tutes the chief function of respiration, and the animal spirits, elabo- rated in the brain from the vital spirits and responsible for the functions of motion and sensation. Galen (131 A. D.) demonstrated that the arteries as well as the veins contain blood, but still believed that the chief function of the respiratory movements is to furnish 620 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. pneuma or vital spirits to the heart. This great physiologist noticed also that the air is necessary for combustion as it is for life, and stated his belief that the explanation of one of these acts would be also an explanation of the other. This thought seems to have been accepted by all the physiologists of subsequent times, but it required over sixteen hundred years of investigation before a satis- factory solution was reached. Galen recognized, moreover, that not only does the blood take something of essential importance from the air. — namely, vital spirits, — but it also gives off something to the air that is injurious to the body, a something which he compared to the smoke of combustion and designated as the "fuliginous vapor." If we substitute oxygen for vital spirits and carbon dioxid for fuliginous vapor we realize that the essential problem 'of respiration was already clearly formulated, but could not make further advance until chemical knowledge was more fully developed. Such is the case with some of our physiological problems to-day. Galen also explained satisfactorily the respiratory movements, the action of the muscles of inspiration and expiration, thus destroying the older erroneous theories that the expansion and contraction of the lungs are clue to processes of heating and cooling. Galen's physiology held undisputed sway until the seventeenth century. At that time there arose a school of physiologists, the iatromechanists, who proposed to explain all vital phenomena upon known mechanical principles, — the laws of physics and chemistry. For the mystical view of vital spirits they proposed to substitute a more rational and concrete theory. The blood in the lungs becomes red simply because it is minutely subdivided and shaken, just as a tube of blood becomes red when violently agitated. Thus an effort to be more scientific, to use the exact knowledge of physics, led to the adoption of views which we now know were far more erroneous than the ancient and intrinsically correct conception that the blood receives something from the air in the lungs. In the seventeenth century, however, began those discoveries in chemistry and physiology which eventually led to our present knowledge. Van Belmont (1577-1644) discovered that in the burning of charcoal, the fermentation of wine, and the action of vinegar on chalk a special gas is produced which he called gas sylvestre and which we call carbon dioxid. Robert Boyle (1627- 1691) published a most interesting series of experiments made with the aid of the recently discovered air-pump which demonstrated the correctness of the view held by Galen that the air contains some- thing necessary lor life mid for combustion. Be showed, moreover, thai :ur that had been repeatedly inspired was no longer capable of maintaining life. Robert Booke (1636-1703) introduced a method of artificial respiration by means of a bellows, and demon- HISTORICAL. 621 strated by sending a continuous stream of air through the lungs that the respiratory movements of these organs are in themselves, as a mechanical process, in no wise an essential feature of respiration. John Mayow in 1688-1674 discovered that air is not a simple ele- ment, but contains a definite substance necessary to life and to combustion. He designated this substance as the nitro-aerian vapor or nitrous particles, because he believed that the same substance is present in condensed form, as it were, in common niter, having found that combustion is possible even in a vacuum in the presence of niter. In the eighteenth century, as is shown in the work of the great physiologist, Haller, the theories of respiration were in many respects in a most unsatisfactory state. The new facts that had been discovered made the old views untenable, but were not in themselves sufficient to explain clearly what actually takes place. Such periods of uncertainty and dissatisfaction are frequent enough in the history of science. In 1757 Joseph Black rediscovered carbon dioxid, calling it fixed air, and showed that it is present in expired air. A little later Priestly discovered and isolated oxygen and nitrogen; but, under the influence of an erroneous view of combus- tion that had been advanced by Stahl, was unable to give his discoveries a clear and satisfactory application. The final step in this progress was made by the wonderful work of Lavoisier between the years 1771 and 1780. He made correct analyses of air and of carbon clioxid, he explained combustion as an oxidation with the formation of C02 and H20, he showed that in respiration the same process occurs, and that the blood takes oxygen from the air and gives back to it in expiration the carbon dioxid and water formed by combustion within the body. He gave us the essential facts in the modern theories of respiration and physio- logical oxidations. After Lavoisier the chief positive advances that have been made have been in reference to the condition of the gases in the blood. By means of the gas-pump Magnus (1837) obtained these gases quantitatively and thus procured data which, as Liebig showed, demonstrate that the oxygen is held in the blood, not in simple solution, but in some form of chemical combination, probably with the red corpuscles. Finally it was shown by Stokes and Hoppe-Seyler that the oxygen is held in definite chemical com- bination with the hemoglobin. The nature of the combination of the carbon dioxid in the blood is not yet entirely understood, while the actual nature of physiological oxidations — that is, the part taken by the oxygen in the chemical reactions of living matter— is one of the great problems of nutrition which may need many years for solution. CHAPTER XXXIV. THE ORGANS OF EXTERNAL RESPIRATION AND THE RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. Anatomical Considerations. — Some of the anatomical ar- rangements in the lungs which have an immediate physiological interest may be recalled briefly. The structures of the trachea and bronchi are admirably adapted to their functions as air tubes, in that the walls possess flexibility combined with rigidity. The lining of ciliated epithelium throughout the air passages is of importance, primarily it may be assumed, in removing mucus and foreign material from these passages. The smaller bronchi possess a dis- tinct muscular layer, and, as we shall see, this musculature is under the control of a special set of nerve fibers through whose reflex activity the capacity and resistance of the bronchial system may bo modified. The smallest bronchioles are expanded into a s}>-stem of membranous air cells, and in the walls of these thin sacs the capil- laries of the pulmonary artery are distributed. The great efficiency of this apparatus is evident when one recalls that every one of the infinite number of red corpuscles is exposed separately to the air in the air cells, so that although the time of transit is brief the entire amount of hemoglobin is nearly completely saturated with oxygen. Each lung is enveloped in its own pleural sac. The space between the parietal and the visceral layer of each sac is the so-called pleural cavity, but it must be borne in mind that under all normal conditions this cavity is only potential, — that is, the parietal and visceral layers are everywhere in contact with each other. Under pathological or accidental conditions air or exudations may enter thifi space and form an actual cavity. Along the mid-line of the body and around the roots of the lungs we have the mediastinal spaces lying between the pleural sacs of the two sides, but entirely filled with the various thoracic viscera, such as the heart, aorta and its branches, pulmonary artery and veins, vena1 cava', azvgos vein, trachea, esophagus, thoracic duct, various nerves, and lymph glands. All these organs, therefore, lie outside the lungs. A schematic view of these relations is represented in Fig. 254. The Thorax as a Closed Cavity. The thorax is a cavity entirely shut off from the outside and from the abdominal cavity. In this cavity lie the Lungs and the various viscera enumerated above. The lungs may be considered as two large, membranous sacs, as fi'J2 EXTERNAL RESPIRATION AND RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 623 represented in Fig. 254, the interior of which communicates freely with the outside air through the trachea, glottis, etc., while the outside of the sacs is protected from atmospheric pressure by the walls of the chest. It is to be remembered, of course, that the interior surface of the lungs is multiplied greatly by the subr division into alveoli. It is estimated that the entire inner surface of the lungs amounts to as much as 90 square meters, over one hun- dred times the skin surface of the body. The atmos- pheric pressure on the interior surfaces of the lungs expands these structures under normal conditions until they fill the entire thoracic cavity not occupied by other organs. However the size of the chest cavity varies, that of the lungs must change accord- ingly; so that at all times the lungs fully fill up every part of the cavity not otherwise occupied. If the wall of the thorax is opened at any point so as to make commu- nication with the outside air, or, if the wall of the lung is pierced so that the air can communicate with the pleural cavity from the inside, then at once the lungs shrink in size, since the atmos- pheric pressure is then equalized on the outside and the inside of the sacs. We may consider, therefore, that the thoracic cavity is much larger than the lungs, and that the latter are blown out to fill this cavity by the atmospheric pressure on the inside. The Normal Position of the Thorax — Inspiration and Expira- tion.— During life the size of the thorax is continually changing with the respiratory movements. But the size and position taken at the end of a normal expiration may be regarded as the normal position of the thorax; that is, its position when all of the muscles of respira- tion are at rest, and substantially, therefore, the position of the thorax in the cadaver. Starting from this position, any enlarge- ment of the thorax constitutes an active inspiration, the result of which will be to draw more air into the lungs ; while starting from the normal position any diminution in the size of the thorax constitutes an active expiration, which will drive some air out of the lungs. It is evident, however, that after an active inspiration the Fig. 254. — Schema to indicate the re- lations of the parietal and visceral layers of the pleural sacs, and the position of the me- diastinal space: P, the potential pleural cavity in each sac; M, the mediastinal space: R.L. and L.L., the cavity of the right and the left lung, respectively; T, the trachea. The outlines of the pleura on each side are represented in dotted lines. 624 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. thorax may return passively to its normal position, giving what is known as a passive expiration, — that is, an expiration not caused by muscular effort. So after an active expiration the thorax ma}' return passively to its normal position, giving a passive inspiration. Our normal respiratory movements consist of an active inspiration followed by a passive expiration, Mechanism of the Inspiration. — The chest cavity ma3r be enlarged and an inspiration, therefore, be produced by two methods, — namely, by a contraction of the diaphragm and by an elevation of the ribs. Contraction of the Diaphragm. — From the anatoiii}- of the diaphragm it is evident that its fixed attachment is found in its muscular connections with the lumbar vertebrae, the ribs, and the ensiform cartilage. From these attachments the muscular sheet extends anteriorly along the walls of the thorax and then bends over to form the arch which ends in the central tendon. This latter structure is not entirely free, since it is attached to the pericar- dium of the heart ; but, relatively, it is the movable portion of the diaphragm. Speaking generally, a contraction of the dia- phragmatic muscle draws the central tendon downward toward the abdominal cavity and therefore enlarges the chest in the vertical diameter, while an increase in the thoracic cavity around the periphery of the diaphragm is caused also by the flattening of the muscular arch. Two results follow this movement: The lungs are expanded exactly in proportion as the cavity enlarges. There is, of course, at no time any space between the lungs and the dia- phragm: as the latter moves downward the lungs follow because of the excess of pressure on their interior. Although ordinarily we speak of the new air being sucked into the lungs during this move- ment, it is, of course, strictly speaking, forced in by the pressure of i he outside atmosphere. ( )n the oilier hand, the descent of the dia- phragm raises the pressure in the abdominal cavity. This cavity is entirely full of viscera and for mechanical purposes may be regarded as being full of liquid. The rise; of pressure is transmitted throughout the abdomen and causes the abdominal wall to protrude. Inspiration caused by a contraction of the diaphragm is therefore spoken of either as diaphragmatic respiration or as abdominal respiration, the latter term having reference to the visible effect on the abdominal walls. In -it ron» contractions of the diaphragm the heart also is pulled downward, and if the movement is forced the lower ribs may be pulled inward to some extent. This last effect would diminish the size of the thorax and therefore wonld tend to antagonize the inspiratory action of the diaphragm, and other muscles are appar- ently broughl into play to prevent this result. Asstated below, the EXTERXAL RESPIRATION AND RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 625 Fig. 255. — Sixth dorsal vertebra and rib. — (Reickert.) quadratus lumborum and the serratus posticus inferior may have this function of fixating the lower ribs in violent inspirations. The diaphragmatic muscle is innervated on each side by the corresponding phrenic nerve. This nerve arises in the neck from the fourth and fifth cervical spinal nerves, and passes downward in the chest in the mediastinal space, lying close to the heart in part of its course. Section of this nerve paralyzes, of course, the diaphragm on the cor- responding side. Elevation of the Ribs. — As a necessary result of the structure of the bony thorax, every elevation of the ribs must cause an enlarge- ment of the thoracic cavity in the dorsoventral and the lateral diam- eters. We are justified in saving that every muscle whose contrac- tion causes an elevation of the ribs is an inspiratory muscle. This result is due, in the first place, to the slant of the ribs. Each rib is attached to the spinal column at two points: the head to the body of the vertebra and the tubercle to the transverse process. The up-and- down movements of the ribs may be re- garded as rotations around ah axis joining these two points, — that is, each point in the rib as it moves up or down describes a circle around this axis (see Fig. 255). If our ribs were set upon the vertebral col- umn so that the plane of the rib formed a right angle with the column, then ever)' movement of the rib up or down would decrease the size of the thorax and there- fore cause an expiration. As a matter of fact, however, the ribs slant downward, so that if elevated the sternal end is car- ried farther away from the sternum and the chest is enlarged in the dorsoventral direction (see Fig. 256). Moreover, as the rib moves upward there 40 — 1*£ S Fig. 256. — Diagram to il- lustrate the effect of the slant of the ribs: S, The spinal col- umn; a, the position of the rib in normal expiration; (a') its position (exaggerated) in inspiration (the distance be- tween the spinal column and the sternum (st.), the antero- posterior or dorsoventral di- ameter of the chest is in- creased). Any movement from the position a' would cause an expiration. 626 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. is an obvious enlargement of the chest in the lateral diameter. This result may be referred to two causes: In the first place, the axis of the rotation of the ribs, — that is, the line joining the head and the tubercle of the rib is inclined downward so that the plane of rotation, which is, of course, at right angles to this axis, will be inclined outward. As the rib is moved upward, therefore, it must also move outward. Secondly the cartilaginous ends of the ribs are fixed at the sternum so that as they move upward and outward they will be twisted or everted somewhat in the middle, with a torsion of the cartilaginous ends. The Muscles of Inspiration. — In addition to the diaphragm, all muscles attached to the thorax whose contraction causes an elevation of the ribs must be classed as inspiratory muscles. In regard to this latter group the action of some of them is either evident from their anatomical attachments, or the muscles may be stimulated directly and the effect of their contraction be noted. In other cases, however, it is necessary to make use of the method first suggested by Newell Martin, — namely, the determination whether the contraction of the muscle in respiration occurs simul- taneously with that of the diaphragm or alternately with it. In the former case it is inspiratory, in the latter expiratory. The following muscles may be classed as inspiratory: Levatore costarum. They arise from the transverse processes of the seventh cervical and first to eleventh thoracic vertebra? and are inserted into the next rib or the second rib below. Intercostales externi muscles. They lie in the inter- costal spaces extending from the lower edge of one rib to the upper edge of the rib below; they slant downward and toward the mid-line. These muscles have been assigned different functions by different authors, but the experiments made by Hough,* using the method of Martin described above, show that they are inspiratory. It was found that in the dog they contract synchronously with the diaphragm. The same authors find that the intercartilaginous portions of the internal intercostals are also inspiratory. The scaleni — anterior, medius, and posterior — arise from the transverse of the cervical vertebrae and arc inserted into the first and second ril)S. M . slrrno-clcido-mfisloidcus extends from the mastoid process to the sternum and sternal extremity of the clavicle. M. pectoralis minor extends from the coracoid process of the scapula, to the anterior surface of the second to the fifth rib. M. scrratus posticus superior extends from the spinous processes of the lower cervical and upper dorsal vertebras to the second to fifth rib. The Muscles of Expiration. -Expiration — that is, diminution * Hough, "Studies from the Biological Laboratory. John Hopkins i'iuvit it.v." r>, oi, 1893, and Bergendal and Bergman, " Skandinavwchea Archiv f. Phy iobgie," 7, L78, 1896. EXTERNAL RESPIRATION AND RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 627 iu size of the thorax — may also be produced in two ways: First, by forcing the diaphragm farther into the thoracic cavity. This result is obtained, not by any direct action of the diaphragm, but by contracting the muscular walls of the abdomen, the external and internal oblique, the rectus, and the transversus. The contraction of these muscles, which form what has been called the abdominal press, raises the pressure in the abdomen and this, acting upon the under surface of the diaphragm, forces it up into the thorax, pro- vided the glottis is open. If the glottis is kept closed firmly the increased abdominal pressure is felt mainly upon the pelvic organs, and this effect is observed in micturition, defecation, and parturition. Second, by depressing the ribs. The muscles which may be sup- posed to exert this action are as follows: M. inter xostales interni. The expiratory action of these muscles, so far as the interosseous portion is concerned, was first definitely shown by Martin, who proved that when they contract they act alternately with the dia- phragm.* M. triangularis sterni or them, transversus thoracis is found on the interior of the thorax on the anterior wall. Its fibers pass from the sternum, running upward and outward, to be inserted into the third to sixth rib. The expiratory action of this muscle was demonstrated by Hough according to the method of Martin. M. iliocostalis lumborum. The anatomical attachments of this muscle are such as would enable it to depress the ribs; but its functional activity in expiration has not been demonstrated. The m. serratus posticus inferior and m. quadratus lumborum are both placed anatomically, especially the former, so that their contractions serve to depress the ribs. It has been suggested, however, that they may act in forced inspirations so as to antagonize the ten- dency of the diaphragm to pull the lower ribs inward. Whether they really act with the diaphragm or alternately with it can only be determined by actual experiment. Quiet and Forced Respiratory Movements; Eupnea and Dyspnea. — Our respiratory movements vary much in amplitude, and the muscles actually involved differ naturally with the extent of the movement. In general, we distinguish two different forms of breathing movements. The ordinary quiet respirations, made without obvious effort, form a condition of respiration designated as eupnea. Difficult or labored breathing is known as dyspnea. It is impossible to draw a sharp line between the two. There are many degrees of dyspnea, and doubtless in quiet breathing the amplitude of the movements may vary considerably before they become distinctly dyspneic. In all conditions of eupnea the chief point to bear in mind is that the expiration is entirely passive. * Martin and Hartwell, "Journal of Physiology," 2, 24, 1879. 6*2$ PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. The inspiration in man is made by the diaphragm alone or by the diaphragm together with some action of the levatores costarum and the external intercostals. At the end of the inspiration the ribs and diaphragm are brought back to the normal position by purely physical forces, — the elasticity of the distended abdominal wall, the elasticity of the expanded kings, the weight and torsion of the ribs, etc. As soon as the breathing movements become at all forced the action of the above-named inspirator}' muscles is in- creased in intensity, and the other inspiratory muscles, all elevators of the ribs, come into play. Quiet breathing in man at least is mainly diaphragmatic or abdominal, while dvspneic breathing is characterized by a greater action of the elevators of the ribs. When dyspnea reaches a certain stage the expiration also becomes active or forced. The expiratory act is hastened by a contraction of the abdominal muscles or of the depressors of the ribs, and indeed the action of these muscles may compress the chest beyond its normal position, so that the expiration is followed by a passive inspiration which brings the chest to its normal position before the next active inspiration begins. Costal and Abdominal Types of Respiration. — These two types of respiration are based upon the character of the inspiratory movement. An inspiration in which the movement of the abdomen, due to contraction of the diaphragm, is the chief or only feature belongs to the abdominal type. An inspiration in which the eleva- tion of the ribs is a noticeable factor belongs to the costal type. Hutchinson, who introduced tins nomenclature,* laid emphasis chiefly upon the order of the movements. In the abdominal type the abdomen bulges outward first, and this is followed by a movement of the thorax; the movement spreads from the abdomen to the thorax, and, "like a wave, is lost over the thoracic region." In costal breathing the upper ribs move first and the abdomen second. The terms are meant to apply chiefly to human respiration and have aroused interest in connection with the facl thai in quiet breathing in the erect posture the respiration of man belongs to the abdominal type and that of woman to the costal type. It lias been a (|iiesfion whether I his difference is a genuine gexual distinction or depends simply upon differences in dress. Hutchinson inclined to the view that if forms what we should call a secondary sexual characteristic, and that its physio- Logical value for woman lies in I he fact that provision is thus made, as it were, against the period of pregnancy, lie states (hat in twenty-four young girls examined between the ages of eleven and fourteen the eosfal type was present, although none of them had * See Hutchinson, article on "Thorax," Todd's "Cyclopaedia of Anat* omy iui'I Physiology, " L849. EXTERNAL RESPIRATION AND RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 629 worn tight dress. Later observers, however (Mays, Kellogg, and others), state that Indian and Chinese women who have not worn tight dress exhibit the abdominal type, and the same statement is made regarding civilized white women who habitually wear loose clothing. It would appear, therefore, that the assumption of the costal type by women in general is due to the hindrance offered by the clothing to the movements of the abdomen. From an exami- nation of four hundred and seven cases Fitz * concludes that when the restricting effect of dress is removed there is little or no differ- ence in the type of respiration in the two sexes. The natural type is one in which " the movement is fairly equally balanced between chest and abdomen, the abdominal being somewhat in excess." When the respiration becomes dyspneic it takes on a distinctly costal type, and Fitz and others have shown that for an equal in- crease in girth the thoracic movements cause a greater enlargement of the lungs. Accessory Respiratory Movements. —In addition to the mus- cles whose action directly enlarges or diminishes the capacity of the thorax certain other muscles connected with the air passages con- tract rhythmically with the inspirations, and may be designated properly as accessory muscles of inspiration. The muscles es- pecially concerned are those controlling the size of the glottis and the opening of the external nares. At each inspiration the elevators of the wings of the nose come into play. This movement occurs in normal breathing in many animals, such as the rabbit and horse, and in some men, while in dyspneic breathing it is invariably present. The useful result of the movement is to reduce the resis- tance to the inflow of air. So in many animals the glottis is dilated at each inspiration by the contraction of the posterior crico-aryte- noid muscles, and in man also this movement is evident when the breathing is at all forced. The useful result in this case also is a reduction in the resistance offered to the inflow of air. The Registration of the Rate and Amplitude of the Respira- tory Movements. — Many methods are employed to register the rate or amplitude of the respiratory movements. Upon man the amplitude may be measured directly by a tape placed at different levels to ascertain the increase in girth, or it may be recorded by some form of lever or tambour applied to the chest or abdomen. A convenient instrument for this purpose is the pneumograph described by Marey, which is illustrated and described in Fig. 257. In animal experimentation the various methods that are employed may be classified under four heads: (1) Methods in which the change in circumference or diameter of the chest or abdomen is recorded. (2) Methods in which the change of pressure in the air * Fitz, "Journal of Experimental Medicine/' 1, 1896. 630 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. passages is recorded. In these methods a tube may be inserted into one of the nostrils for instance, and then connected to a tambour the lever of which makes its record on a kymographion, or if the animal is tracheotomized a side tube upon the tracheal cannula may be connected to a tambour. This method indicates well the rate of movement and the relative amplitude, but has the defect that it Fig. 257. — Figure of Marey's pneumograph. — (Verdin.) The instrument consists of a tambour (/), mounted on a flexible metal plate (p). By means of the bands c and c the metal plate is tied to the chest. Any increase or decrease in the size of the chest will then affect the tambour by the lever arrangement shown in the figure. These changes in the tambour are transmitted through the tube r as pressure changes in the contained air to a second tambour (not shown in the figure) which records them upon a smoked drum. does not record the pause, if any, at the end of inspiration or ex- piration. A modification of this method that permits an accurate record of the amplitude and duration of the movements consists in connecting the trachea or nostrils with a large bottle of air. The animal breathes into and out of the bottle, and the corresponding Fig. 268. Curve of QOimal respiratory movements. — (Marc//.) Curve A, full line, repret«Mii the movements when the re piration is entirely normal. Downstroke, Inspira- tion; upstroke, expiration. Curved dotted line, represents the increased amplitude "f the movement , light dy.spn i ..I by l>n-;i I liing through a narrow tube. variations in pressure are recorded by a tambour also connected with the interior of the bottle. (3) Methods in which the change of pressure in the thoracic cavity 18 recorded. This end may be reached by inserting a cannula into the thoracic wall so that its opening lies in the pleural cavity, or, more simply, a catheter or SOUnd Connected .'it I In- Other end to a tambour may be passed down EXTERNAL RESPIRATION AND RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 631 the esophagus until its end lies in the intrathoracic portion. Variations in pressure in the mediastinal space synchronous with the respiratory movements affect the esophagus and through it the sound. (4) Methods in which the movements of the dia- phragm are recorded either by a tambour or lever thrust between the diaphragm and liver, or by hooks attached directly to muscular slips of the diaphragm. Registration of the movements in man during quiet breathing give us such a record as is seen in Fig. 258. It will be seen that the inspiration (descend- ing limb) is followed at once by an expiration, as we should expect, since, as soon as the inspiratory muscles cease to act, the physical factors mentioned above at once tend to bring the chest back to its normal position. The expiration (ascending limb) is at first rapid and toward the end very gradual, so that there is al- most a condition of rest, — an expira- tory pause. The Volumes of Air Respired and the Capacity of the Lungs. — The volume of air respired varies, of course, with the extent of the move- ments and the size of the individual. This volume may be determined readily in any given case by means of a spirometer, — a form of gasometer adapted to this purpose. The con- struction of this apparatus is repre- sented in Fig. 259. It consists of a graduated cylinder (A) and a receiver (B) filled with water. The cylinder A is counterbalanced by a weight (g) so as to move up and down in the water of B with the least possible re- sistance. The tube C passes through the wall of B and ends in the interior of A above the level of the water. The free end of this tube is connected with the mouth or nose. When one breathes through this tube the expired air passes into A, which rises from the water to receive it. If A is graduated the amount of air breathed out may be measured directly. The following terms are used: Vital capacity. By vital capacity is meant the quantity of air that can be breathed _ Fig. 259— Wintrieh 's modifi- cation of Hutchinson's spirometer. — (Reichert.) 632 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. out by the deepest possible expiration after making the deepest possible inspiration. It gives a rough measure of lung capacity, and is used in gymnasiums and physical examinations for this pur- The actual amount varies with the individual; an average figure for the adult man is 3700 c.c. Tidal air. By this term is meant the amount of air breathed out in a normal quiet expiration. A similar amount is breathed in. of course, in the previous inspira- tion, and the term tidal air designates the amount of air that flows in and out of the lungs with each quiet respiratory movement. Here, again, there are individual variations. The average figure for the adult man is 500 c.c. The complemented air. This term designates the amount of air that can be breathed in over and above the tidal air by the deepest possible inspiration. It is estimated at 1600 c.c. The supplemental air. By this term is meant the amount of air that can be breathed out, after a quiet expiration, by the most forcible expiration. It is equal also to 1600 c.c. It is evident that the complemental air plus the supplemental air plus the tidal air constitute the vital capacity. The residual air. After the most forcible expiration the lungs are far from being entirely collapsed. The volume of air that remains behind, after the sup- plemental air has been driven out, is known as the residual air. The amount of this air has been estimated directly on the cadaver (Hermann). The thorax was first pressed into a position of forced expiration; the trachea was then ligated, the chest opened, the lungs removed and their volume estimated by the amount of water dis- placed when they were immersed. The average result from such estimations was, in round numbers, 1000 c.c. Under conditions of normal breathing the reserve supply of air in the lungs is equal to the residual air plus the supplemental air, — that is, 2600 c.c. Mini- mal air. When the thorax is opened the lungs collapse, driving out the supplemental and residual air, hut not quite completely. Before the air cells arc entirely emptied the small bronchi leading to them collapse and I heir walls adhere with sufficient force to entrap a little air in I lie alveoli. It is on this account thai the excised lungs float in water ami arc designated as lights by the butcher. The small amount of air caught in this way is designated as the minimal air. In the fetus before birth the lungs are entirely solid, but after birth, if respirations are made, the lungs do not collapse completely on account of the capture of the minimal air. Whether or not the lungS will float ha.- constituted, therefore, one of the farts used in medicolegal cases to determine if a child was stillborn. The lungs during life may, under certain conditions, again become in parts entirely solid. If any of the alveoli become completely shut- off from the brathea, by an accident or by pathological conditions, the air caughl in them may be completely absorbed, after a certain interval, by the circulating blood. EXTERNAL RESPIRATION AND RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 633 The Size of the Bronchial Tree and the Ventilation of the Lungs. — Since the reserve supply of air in the lungs may amount to 2600 c.c, while the new air breathed in at each inspiration amounts to only 500 c.c, it would seem at first that the alveolar air is not very efficiently renewed by a quiet inspiration. The actual amount of ventilation effected depends on the capacity of the bronchial tree, sometimes known as the "dead space" of the lungs, since the air filling this space is not useful in the respiratory processes. According to observations founded partly on measurements of casts of the tree and partly upon physiological determinations made by breathing air poor in oxygen, it would seem that this volume may be reckoned at 140 c.c* At each inspiration, there- fore, at least 360 c.c. of air penetrate into the alveoli, and if evenly disseminated through the lungs add about -fa to the volume of each alveolus. Once in the alveoli, diffusion must tend to spread the tidal air rapidly, and that this occurs is shown by an interesting experiment performed by Grehant. He breathed in 500 c.c of hydrogen instead of air and then examined the amounts of hy- drogen breathed out in successive expirations. Only 170 c.c. were recovered in the first expiration, 180 c.c in the second, 41 in the third, and 40 in the fourth. Artificial Respiration. — In laboratory experiments artificial respiration is employed frequently after the use of curare ; when it is necessary to open the chest; after cessation of respirations from overdoses of chloroform or ether, etc The method used in almost all cases is the reverse of the normal procedure, — that is, the lungs are expanded by positive pressure (pressure in excess of atmos- pheric). A bellows or blast worked by hand or machinery is con- nected with the trachea and the lungs are dilated by rhythmical strokes. Provision is made for the escape of expired air by the use of valves or by a side hole in the tracheal cannula. Numerous forms of respiration pumps have been devised for this purpose. In cases of suspended respiration in human beings from drown- ing, electrical shocks, pressure upon the medulla, etc., it is necessary to use artificial respiration in order to restore normal breathing. Bellows ordinarily cannot be used in such cases. Some method must be employed to expand and contract the chest alternately, and several different ways have been devised. The Marshall Hall method consists in placing the subject face down and rolling the body from this to a lateral position, making some pressure upon the back while in the prone position. The Sylvester method, which is frequently used, consists in raising the arms above the head and then bringing them down against the sides of the chest so as to compress the latter. The Howard method consists in simply com- * See Loewy, " Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 58, 416. 634 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. pressing the lower part of the chest while the subject is in a supine position. Schaefer. who has recently compared these different methods, suggests one of his own, which seems to be effective, saves labor, and is less injurious to the subject.* He describes it as fol- lows: "It consists in laying the subject in the prone posture, preferably on the ground, with a thick folded garment underneath the chest and epigastrium. The operator puts himself athwart or at the side of the subject, facing his head (see Fig. 260) and places his hands on each side over the lower part of the back (lowest ribs). He then slowly throws the weight of his body forward to bear upon ^^^ r PK' j _ j «-~.^r T^B Unf Fig. 260.- -Shows the position to be adopted for effecting artificial respiration in cases of drowning. — {Schaefer.) his own arms, and thus presses upon the thorax of the subject and forces air out of the lungs. This being effected, he gradually re- laxes the pressure by bringing his own body up again to a more erect position, but without moving the hands." These movements are repeated quite regularly at a rate of twelve to fifteen times a minute until normal respiration begins or the possibility of its restoration is abandoned. A half-hour or more may be required before normal breathing movements start. ♦Schaefer, " Medico-chirurgical Transactions," London, vol. lxxxvii, 1904. CHAPTER XXXV. THE PRESSURE CONDITIONS IN THE LUNGS AND THORAX AND THEIR INFLUENCE UPON THE CIRCULATION. In considering the pressure changes in respiration the distinction between the pressure in the thorax outside the lungs and the pres- sure within the lungs and air passages must be kept clearly in mind. The pressure in the thoracic cavity outside the lungs may be designated as the intrathoracic pressure; it is the pressure exerted upon the heart, great blood- vessels, thoracic duct, esopha- gus, etc. The pressure in the interior of the lungs and air passages may be designated as intrapulmonic pressure. The relations of the two pressures with reference to the outside atmosphere is indicated sche- matically in Fig. 261. The Intrapulmonic Pres- sure and its Variations. — The air passages and the alveoli of the lungs are in free communi- cation with the external air; consequently in every position of rest, whether at the end of inspiration or expiration, the pressure in these cavities is equal to that of the atmos- phere outside. During the act of inspiration, however, the in- trapulmonic pressure falls tem- porarily below that of the atmosphere, — that is, during the inflow of air. The extent to which the pressure falls depends naturally upon the rapidity and amplitude of the inspiratory movement and upon the size of the opening to the exterior. The narrowest portion of the air passages is the glottis; consequently the variations in pres- sure below this point are probably greater than in the pharynx or nasal cavities. If the air passages are abnormally constricted at 635 Fig. 261. —Diagram to illustrate how the pressure of the air is exerted through the lung walls upon the heart {H) and other organs in the mediastinal space. The pres- sure on these organs (intrathoracic pressure) is equal to one atmosphere minus the amount of the opposing pressure exerted by the ex- panded lungs. 636 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. any point the fall of pressure during inspiration -will be correspond- ingly magnified in the parts below the constriction, as happens, for instance, in bronchial asthma, edema of the glottis, cold in the head. etc. Under normal conditions the fall of pressure during a quiet inspiration is not large. Donders determined it in man by connecting a water manometer with one nostril and found that it was equal to — 9 or — 10 nuns, water. At the end of an inspiration, if there is a pause, the pressure within the lungs again rises, of course, to atmospheric. During expiration, on the other hand, the collapse of the chest wall takes place with sufficient rapidity to compress the air somewhat during its escape and cause a temporary rise of pres- sure. In normal expiration Donders estimated this rise as equal to 7 or 8 mms. water. The intrapulmonic pressure may vary greatly from these figures in the positive or negative direction according to the factors mentioned above, especially the intensity of the respira- tory movement and the size of the opening to the exterior. The extreme variations are obtained when the opening to the outside is entirely shut off. When an inspiration or an expiration is made with the glottis firmly closed the pressure in the lungs, of course, rises and falls with the rarefaction or compression of the contained air. A strong inspiration under such conditions may lower the pressure by 30 to 80 mms. of mercury, while a strong expiration raises the pressure by an amount equal to 60 to 100 mms. Hg. In the act of coughing we get a similar result: the strong spasmodic expirations are made with a closed glottis and consequently cause a marked rise in the intrapulmonic pressure. Such great variations in pressure have a marked influence on the heart and the circula- tion, as is explained below. Intrathoracic Pressure. —When a reference is made to the pressure within the thorax, it is the intrathoracic pressure that is meant, — that is, the pressure in the pleural cavity and mediastinal spaces. This pressure, under normal conditions, is always negative, — that is, is always less than one atmosphere. The reason for this ie simply that the lungs are distended to fill the thoracic cavity, and consequently the organs, like the heart, which lie in this cavity outside the lungs, are exposed to a pressure of one atmosphere, minus the force of elastic recoil of the lungs (see Fig,261). Theheart and other intrathoracic organs are protected from the direct pres- sure of the air by the thoracic walls; they are pressed upon, how- ever, through the lungs, but naturally the atmospheric pressure is reduced by an amount equal to the elastic force of the distended lungs. Intrathoracic pressure, in fact, may be denned as intra- pulmonic pressure minus the elastic pull of the lungs, and since; under usual conditions I he int mpiihnnnic pressure is equal to that PRESSURE CONDITIONS IN LUNGS AND THORAX. 637 of the atmosphere, the intrathoracic pressure is less than an atmosphere by an amount equal to the recoil of the lungs. The negative pressure in the thorax is, therefore, equal to the elastic force of the lungs, and is larger the more the lungs are put upon a stretch, — that is, the deeper the inspiration. The amount of this negative pressure has been measured upon both animals and men by two methods: First by Donder's method of attaching a manom- eter to the trachea and then opening the thoracic walls so as to allow the atmosphere to press upon the exterior face of the lungs. In this way the elastic force of the lungs is determined, and, as explained above, this is equivalent to the negative pressure. Second, by thrusting a trocar through the thoracic wall so that its open end may lie in the pleural or mediastinal cavity, the other end being appropriately connected with a manometer. The older observers (Hutchinson) also made experiments upon freshly excised human lungs, determining their elastic force when distended by known amounts of air. The figures obtained by these different methods have shown some variations, but the following quotations give an idea of the average extent of this negative pressure. Heynsius,* making use of the figures obtained by Hutchinson, estimates that in man the negative pressure in the thorax at the end of expiration is — 4.5 mms. Hg, while at the end of an inspiration it is equal to — 7.5 mms. Hg, — a variation during respiration, therefore, of 3 mms. Hg. That is, assuming that the atmospheric pressure is 760 mms. Hg, the conditions of pressure in the thorax and lungs at the end of inspiration and expiration are as follows: At the End of Inspiration. At the End of Expiration. Intrapulmonic pressure . . . 760 mms. Hg. 760 mms. Hg. Intrathoracic pressure .... 752.5 " " 755.5 " " Aron gives results obtained from a healthy man in whom a can- nula was connected directly with the pleural cavity. f From 36 determinations he obtained the average result that at the end of quiet inspiration the negative pressure is — 4.64 mms. Hg and at the end of expiration — 3.02 mms. Hg — results considerably lower than those estimated by Heynsius. It should be borne in mind, however, that these values depend upon the condition of expansion of the chest, — that is, the position of the body and the depth of inspiration. On dogs Heynsius reports as follows: At end of inspiration, — 9.4 mms. Hg; end of expiration, — 3.9 mms. Hg. On rabbits, — 4.5 mms. and — 2.5 mms. Hg. Variations of Intrathoracic Pressure with Forced and Unusual Respirations. — After the most forcible expiration, when the air- * "Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 29, 265, 1882. f Aron, quoted from Emerson, "Johns Hopkins Hospital Reports," 11, 194, 1903. 63S PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. passages are open, the intrathoracic pressure is still negative by a small amount, since the lungs are still expanded beyond what might be called their normal size, — that is, their size when the pres- sure inside and outside is the same. If, however, a forced expira- tion is made with the glottis closed, as in the straining movements of defecation, parturition, etc., then naturally the intrathoracic pressure rises with the intrapulmonary pressure. The increased pressure from the compressed air in the lungs is felt upon the organs in the mediastinal spaces. The large veins especially are affected, and the flow in them is partially blocked, as is shown by the swelling of the veins in the neck outside the thorax. The maintenance of such conditions for a considerable period may seriously affect the circulation. The same general effect is obtained also in attacks of coughing, the violent spasmodic expirations with closed glottis causing a visible venous congestion in the head from the obstruction to the venous flow into the heart. Forcible inspirations, on the other hand, lower the intrathoracic pressure — that is, increase the negativity — whether the glottis is open or closed. When the glottis is freely open and a deep inspiration is made the intrathoracic pressure may fall as much as 30 mms. Hg, — that is, become equal to 730 mms. The lungs being much more expanded exert a corre- spondingly greater elastic force. If the glottis is closed during a deep inspiration then there is little actual expansion of the lungs, but the intrapulmonary pressure falls from the rarefaction of the air in the lungs, and the intrathoracic pressure, of course, falls with it. The Origin of the Negative Pressure in the Thorax. — As is evi- dent from the above explanation, the fact that the pressure in the thorax is less than one atmosphere is due in the long run to the circumstance that the lungs are smaller than the thoracic cavity which they occupy. In the fetus the lungs are solid, and completely fill the thoracic cavity, except for the part occupied by the other organs. It has been a question whether after birth the size of the thoracic cavity is suddenly and permanently increased by the first inspiratory movements, and a negative intrathoracic pressure thus produced at once. The careful experiments of Hermann* seem to have settled this point. He proved that newly-born children between 'he first and the fourth day, show no measurable negative pressure in the thorax, and at Hie eighth day the pressure in the thoracic cavity is less than atmospheric by an amount equal to only — 0.4 mm. 11%. The negative pressure as we find it in the adult is evidently developed gradually, and is due to the fact that the thorax increases in size; more rapidly and to a greater extent than the lungs, so that to fill the cavity the lungs become more and more expanded. It follows, also, from these facts, that the new-born * Hermann, " Archiv f. d, geeammte Physiologic," 30, 276, L883. PRESSURE CONDITIONS IN LUNGS AND THORAX. 639 child has practically no reserve supply of air in the lungs; at each expiration the lungs are entirely emptied (except for the minimal air). The ventilation of the lung alveoli is correspondingly more perfect than in older persons. Pneumothorax. — When the pleural cavity on either side is opened by any means air enters and causes a greater or less shrinkage of the corresponding lung. This condition of air within the pleural cavity is designated as pneumothorax. It is evident that air may enter the pleural cavity in one of two general ways : By a puncture of the parietal pleura such as may be made by gunshot or stab wounds in the chest, or by a puncture of the visceral pleura, such as may occur, for example, by the rupture of a tubercle in pulmonary tuberculosis, the air in this case entering from the alveoli of the lungs. From the physical conditions involved it is evident that if the opening into the pleural cavity is kept patent then the lung will collapse completely and eventually will become entirely solid, since the small amount of entrapped minimal air will be absorbed by the blood. The other lung, the heart, etc., will also be displaced somewhat from their normal position by the unusual pressure. If, however, the opening is closed, then the air in the pleural cavity may be absorbed completely by the circulating blood and the lung again expand as this absorption takes place. In human beings pneumothorax occurs most frequently in conditions of disease, particularly pulmonary tuberculosis, and the air in the thorax is associated also with a liquid effusion, this combination being designated sometimes as hydropneumothorax.* The Aspiratory Action of the Thorax. — The negative pres- sure prevailing in the thoracic cavity must affect the organs in the mediastinal space. The intrathoracic portion of the esophagus, for instance, is exposed, at times of swallowing at least, to a full atmosphere of pressure on its interior, while on its exterior it is under the diminished intrathoracic pressure. This difference tends to dilate the tube and may aid in the act of swallowing. The main effect of the difference in pressure is felt, however, upon the flow of lymph and blood, especially the latter. The large veins in the neck and axilla are under the pressure of an atmosphere exerted through the skin, and the same is true for the inferior cava in the abdomen. But the superior and inferior cavse and the right auricle are under a pressure less than one atmosphere. This difference in pressure must act as a constant favoring condition to the flow of blood to the heart. The difference is markedly increased at each inspiration; so that at each such act there is an increase in the velocity and volume of the flow to the heart, — an effect which is * See Emerson, "Pneumothorax," Johns Hopkins Hospital Reports, 11, 1, 1903. 640 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. usually referred to as the aspiratory action of the thorax. At each inspiration blood is " sucked " from the extrathoracic into the intra- thoracic veins. So far as the inferior cava is concerned, this effect is augmented by the simultaneous increase in abdominal pressure. For as the diaphragm descends it raises the pressure in the ab- domen as it lowers the pressure in the thorax. The two fac- tors co-operate in forcing more blood from the abdominal to the thoracic portion of the cava. There should be, of course, a similar effect, but in the opposite direction, upon the flow in the arteries. Each inspiration should retard the arterial outflow from the aorta into its extrathoracic branches. As a matter of fact, this effect probably does not take place. The arteries are thick walled and are distended by a high internal pressure, so that the small change of pressure of three or four millimeters of mercury during inspiration is probably incapable of influencing the caliber of the arteries, while it has a distinct effect upon the thin-walled veins, whose internal pressure is very small. The changes in intra- thoracic pressure during respiration must affect the blood-flow also in the pulmonary circuit, the flow from the right to the left side of the heart. This effect is manifested in the so-called respiratory waves of blood-pressure which may be discussed briefly in this connection. Respiratory Waves of Blood-pressure. — When a record is taken of the blood-pressure the tracing shows waves, unless the n sspi rat ory movements are very shallow, which are synchronous with the respiratory movements (see Fig. 262). When the respiration is dyspneic the waves of pressure are very marked. To ascertain the exact relations of these variations to the phases of respiration it is necessary to make simultaneous tracings of blood-pressure and respiration movements with the recording pens properly superposed. In the dog ii is found that the blood-pressure falls slightly at the beginning of inspiration, but rises during the rest- of the act (Kig. 263). At the beginning of expiration the pressure continues to rise for a time and then falls during most of this phase. On the whole, therefore, the effect of inspiration, its final effect, is to cause a rise of arterial pressure, while the c\'U'c\ of expiration is to cause a fall. The relationship of the two curves varies in other animals, depend- ing, among other things, on the rapidity of the respirations; but, since most of the experimental work has been done upon the dog, our attention may be confined to the relationship shown by this animal. Two general explanations may he given for these respira- tory waves: first, that they are due to an activity of the vaso- constrictor '-enter synchronous with thai of the respiratory center. Second, that they are due to variations in the amount, of blood sent out from the heart into the aorta, this variation, in turn, being due PRESSURE CONDITIONS IN LUNGS AND THORAX. 641 to the mechanical changes in pressure during respiration and their effect on the blood-flow, aided also by the fact that the heart beats more rapidly during inspiration. This second general point of view- has been adopted in physiology, and to verify it numerous experi- ments have been made upon lungs placed in an artificial thorax in which the conditions of pressure could be varied at will.* As the out- come of this work, the following results have been accepted in expla- %N^^\^^^ ^A^^ Fig. 262. — Respiratory waves of blood-pressure. Typical blood-pressure record as taken with a mercury manometer: Bp the blood-pressure record, shows the separate heart beats and the larger respiratory waves, each of which comprises six to seven heart beats. nation of the occurrence of the respiratory waves of blood-pressure : (1) During inspiration there is an increased flow of blood into the right auricle (aspiratory action of inspiration). (2) During inspira- tion the capacity of the blood-vessels in the lungs is increased and also the velocity of the flow; consequently there is an increased * For discussion and literature see de Jager, " Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 20, 426, 1S79, and 27, 152, 1882; also "Journal of Physiology, " 7,130.41 642 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. volume of blood flowing through the lungs during inspiration. The increased capacity of the lung capillaries during the expansion of the lungs was shown experimentally by Heger and Spehl. They opened the anterior mediastinum without wounding the pleura and proved that if the lungs are tied off at the end of inspiration they con- tain more blood than when tied off at the end of expiration. The in- creased velocity of the blood-flow through the lungs during inspira- tion is explained by the fact that the greater negative pressure affects the thin-walled pulmonary veins more than the pulmonary artery ; consequently the head of pressure driving the blood through the lungs, — that is, the difference in pressure between the blood in the pulmonary artery and veins — is increased. These data explain satisfactorily the general fact regarding the respiratory waves, — ■ namely, that during inspiration there is a rise of aortic pressure due to a greater output of blood from the heart, and during expiration Fig. 263. — Diagram to represent the time relation between the respiratory waves of blood-preesure and the respiratory movements (dog)! A represents the blood-pressure record, showing the heart -heats and the larger respiratory waves. H represents a simul- taneous record of the respiratory movements. At the beginning of inspiration there is a fall of blood-pressure, but the final and main effect is a rise. At the beginning of expi- ration there is a rise of pressure, but the final and main effect is a fall. the reverse. To account for the subsidiary fact that at the begin- ning of inspiration the pressure falls and at the beginning of expira- tion it rises for a time two explanations are offered. De Jagcr refers these temporary effects to the changes in capacity of the blood- bed in the lungs. At the cud of inspiration there is a certain ca- pacity of the bed; when expiration comes on, the lungs shrink, the capacity oft he blood-vessels is thereby diminished, and con,se<|iient]y .some blood is squeezed out of the lungs in the direction of least resistance, -that is, toward the left auricle. This accounts for the initial rise of pressure during expiration. At the beginning of inspi- ration, on the ot her hand, the sudden increase in capillary capacity in th'e lungs retards for a moment the flow of blood to the left, auricle, and thus accounts for the temporary fall of pressure. Tigerstedt,* on the other hand, finds that shutting off the entire circulation of one lung may have little or no influence upon the pressure in the * See i iger tedt, " Ergebniese der Physiologie, " vol. ii, part h, 560, 1903. PRESSURE CONDITIONS IN LUNGS AND THORAX. 643 systemic circulation, and therefore doubts whether small changes in the capacity of the lung vessels can have any distinct effect on the inflow into the left auricle. He thinks that the main factor is the increased flow of blood to the right auricle during inspiration, and that this increased amount is then passed on to the left auricle and ventricle, but that this takes some little time, so that the true effect Ox inspiration is not felt in the aorta at the very beginning of the act. This delay may vary in different animals and may account for the fact that in some animals there is an apparent inversion of the relations to respiration, the aortic pressure falling throughout inspiration and rising during expiration. The increased rate of heart beat during inspiration varies as to its degree in different individuals. It has been shown by Fredericq that this change occurs when the chest is widely opened and the respiratory movements can have no mechanical effect upon the heart. He suggests, therefore, that the accelerated pulse during inspiration is due to an associated activity in the nerve centers of the medulla. When the inspiratory center discharges its efferent impulses into the phrenic nerves it also sends impulses by a sort of overflow into the neighboring cardio-inhibitory center. This latter cen- ter is, thereby, partially inhibited, its tonic effect on the heart is diminished, and the rate of the heart is increased. In artificial respiration carried out by means of a bellows — that is, by expanding the lungs with positive pressure — all the conditions of pressure in inspiration and expiration are reversed. During such an inspiration the flow of blood to the right heart, and through the lungs to the left heart, is decreased. Respiratory waves of pressure are present under such conditions, but the rela- tions of rise and fall to the phases of respiration are reversed. CHAPTER XXXVI. THE CHEMICAL AND PHYSICAL CHANGES IN THE AIR AND THE BLOOD CAUSED BY RESPIRATION. The Inspired and the Expired Air. — The inspired air, atmos- pheric air, varies in composition in different places. The essential constituents from a physiological standpoint are the oxygen, nitrogen, and carbon clioxid. The new elements — argon, krypton, etc. — have not been shown to have any physiological significance, and are included with the nitrogen. The accidental constituents of the air vary with the locality. In average figures, the composi- tion of this air is, in volume per cent.: nitrogen, 79; oxygen, 20.96; carbon clioxid, 0.04. The expired air varies in composition with the depth of the expiration and, of course, with the composition of the air inspired. Under normal conditions the expired air contains, in volume per cent. : nitrogen, 79; oxygen, 16.02; carbon dioxid, 4.38. In passing once into the lungs the air, therefore, gains 4.34 volumes of carbon dioxid to each hundred, and loses 4.94 volumes of oxygen. n. o. co2. Inspired 79 20.96 0.04 Expired 79 1 hi which d represents in liters the delivery of fresh air per hour; e, the amount of C02 expired per hour in liters; and r the ratio of permissible vitiation of the air by C02. Assuming this 548 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. latter factor, in accordance with the above statement, to be equal to 0.02 per cent, and e to be equal to 20 liters per hour (500 X 0.04 X 17 X 60), the value of d is equal to 100,000 liters of air per hour for each person. The rapidity of renewal of air will depend naturally upon the cubic space allotted to each individual. The smaller this space, the more ample must be the ventilation. The following figures* give an idea of the values adopted for dif- ferent conditions. Amount of Ventilation Cubic Space per per Hour per Person Person in Cubic in Cubic Meters. Meters. Hospitals 60-100 30-50 Prisons 50 25 rectories 60-100 30-50 Barracks 30-50 15-25 Theaters 40-50 20-25 Halls and assembly rooms 30-60 15-30 Schools ' 15-20 7.5-10 Classrooms for adults 25-30 12-15 The amount of cubic space allowed is based, it will be noted, upon the supposition that the air is twice completely renewed by Ventila- tion during the course of an hour. The Gases of the Blood. — The gases that are contained in the blood are oxygen, carbon dioxid, and nitrogen. These gases may be extracted completely and in a condition for quantitative analysis by means of some form of gas-pump. The principle of most of the gas-pumps used in the physiological laboratories is the same. The apparatus is arranged so that the blood to be examined is brought into a vacuum while kept at the temperature of the body. Under these conditions all of the oxygen and nitrogen and part of the car- bon dioxid are given off and may be collected by suitable means. A port ion of t he carbon dioxid present in the blood is in such stable combination thai to remove if it may be necessary to add some dilute acid, such as phosphoric acid. This portion of the emboli dioxid is designated in this connection as t he fixed carbon dioxid. The principle of the gas pump may be explained most easily by describing the simple form devised byGrehant. The essential parts of this pump are represented in I ilt. 264. The mercury pump consists of two bulbs, one mov- able (M), the other fixed (F). 1/ may be raised and lowered by the windlass (]'). Above /•', there i- a three way stopcock (m) by means of which tin; chamber F may be pul into communical ion with the outi ide air by way of ( ', or with the bulb B, which L bo contain the blood, or may be shut off com- pletely, [f M i- raised so as to fill F entirely, and t Ik- stopcock m is shul off, then on lowering .1/ the mercury will How into it, leaving a, perfect vacuum in /'', since the di tance between F and .1/ is greater than the barometric height. If the topcock /» i- turned o a to throw /*' into communication with /', the chamber of I In- la tier is brought under the influence of the vac- uum and any ease thai it may contain will he distributed bet, ween /.' and I li topcock m i again turned off and .1/ is raised the gases in F will be condensed at it- upper end, and by turning the stopcock m properly these may be forced to the oul ide bj way of C or may be collected, if de- ♦ Taken from Bergey, "The Principles of Hygiene," 1904. CHANGES IN AIR AND BLOOD IN RESPIRATION. 649 sired, in a burette filled with mercury and inverted over the opening from F contained in the bottom of C. In performing an experiment the flask B, which is to contain the blood, is connected with F, as shown in the figure, all joints being protected from leakage by a seal of water outside, as shown at h, which represents a piece of wide rubber tubing filled with water so as to protect a joint between two pieces of glass tubing. B is next exhausted completely by raising and lowering M a number of times in the Avay described above until on throwing B into communication with a vacuum in F no further gas is given off. The last particles of air may be driven out from B by boil- ing a little water in it. After a complete vacuum has been established in B Fig. 264.— Gas pump for extracting the gases of blood (Grehant) : M and F, The mercury receivers; P, the windlass for raising and lowering M ; m, a three-way stopcock protected by a seal of mercury or water; C, a cup with mercury over which the receiving eudiometer is placed to collect the gases; B, the bulb in which, after a vacuum is made, the blood is introduced by the graduated syringe, S. By means of the stopcock m the vacuum in F , caused by the fall of the mercury, can be placed in communi- cation with B. After the gases have diffused over into F, M is raised, and when the stop- cock m is properly turned these gases are driven out through C into the receiving tube. The operation is repeated until no more gas is given off from B. a measured amount of blood is introduced from a graduated syringe, S, as represented in the figure. This blood must be taken directly from the vessels of the animal and be introduced into B at once. B is kept immersed in water at the temperature of the body, and the bulb M is now raised and lowered a number of times so that the gases given off from the blood are drawn over 650 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. into F and then by proper manipulation of the stop-cock are driven into a burette fastened over the opening of the tube in C. To drive off all of the carbon dioxid a little dilute phosphoric acid must be added to the blood in B by means of the syringe, S. The gases thus collected into the burette are tir.-t" measured and are then analyzed for the three important constituents by some of the accepted gasometric methods. The principle involved is to absorb first from the mixture all of the C02 by introducing a solution of sodium or potassium hydrate. The reading of the volume left after this absorption is completed compared with the first reading gives the volume of C02. Next, a freshly made alkaline solution of pyrogallic acid is introduced into the tube. This solution absorbs all of the oxygen, whose volume is thus easily determined. The gas that is left unabsorbed after the action of these two solutions is nitro- gen. "The volumes of gases are reduced, as is the custom, to unit pressure and temperature, — that is, to zero degree centigrade and 760 mms. barometric pressure. A correction must also be made for the tension or pressure exerted by the aqueous vapor in the gases. These corrections are made by means of the following formula: yi- V(B-T) 760 X (1 + 0.003665 1) in which V1 represents the corrected volume, V the volume actually observed, B the barometric height at the time and place of the observation, T the aqueous tension at the temperature of the reading, and t the temperature in degrees centigrade. By means of such methods the gases in the blood have been de- termined. The quantities vary somewhat, of course, with the con- ditions of the animal and with the species of animal. In a quick analysis of dogs' arterial blood made by Pfii'iger the following figures were obtained reckoned in volumes per cent.: O, 22.6; C02, 34.3; N, 1.8. In this case each 100 c.c. of arterial blood contained 22.6 c.c. of O and 34.3 c.c. of C02 measured at 0° C. and 760 mms. Hg. An analysis of human blood (Setschenow) gave closely similar figures; O, 21.6 per cent.; C02, 40.3 per cent.; and N, 1.6 per cent. When the arterial and the venous bloods are compared it is found that the venous blood has more carbon dioxid and less oxygen. Average figures showing the difference in composition are as follows; o. co2. N. Arterial blood 20 38 1.7 Venous blood 1_2 45 L7 Difference 8 7 0 The actual amounts of oxygen and carbon dioxid in the venous blood vary with the nutritive activity of the tissues, and differ therefore in the various organs according to the state of activity of each organ in relation to the volume of its blood supply. This point is well illustrated by some analyses made by Hill and Na- barro* of tin- gases in the venous blood from the brain and the muscle . respectively. Their average results when both tissues were al rest were as follows: OxviiKN. Oaiihon Dioxid. Venous blood from limbs (femoral) ... . 6.84 percent. 45.75 percent " brain (torcular) . . . L3.49 " " 41.65 " " * Hill and Nabarro, "Journal of Physiology," 18, 218, 1895. CHANGES IN AIR AND BLOOD IN RESPIRATION. 651 It will be seen that under similar conditions there is much less oxygen used and carbon dioxicl formed in the brain than in the limbs (muscles). In the former organ the physiological oxidations must either be small compared with those of the muscles, or the brain tissues receive a relatively ample supply of blood, so that the tissue metabolism has less effect upon the blood composition. The- venous blood as it comes to the lungs is a mixture of bloods from different organs, and its composition in gases will be constant only when the conditions of the body are kept uniform. Much work has been clone in physiology to determine the condition in which these various gases are held in the blood. The results obtained show that they are held partly in solution and partly in chemical combination. To understand the part played by each factor and the conditions that control the exchange of gases in the lungs and tissues it is necessary to recall some facts regarding the physical and chemical properties of gases. The Pressure of Gases and the Terms Expressing these Pressures. — The air around us exists under a pressure of one atmosphere and this pressure is expressed usually in terms of the height of a column of mercury that it will support, — namely, a column of 760 mms. Hg, which is known as the normal barometric pressure at sea-level. Air is a mixture of gases, and according to the mechanical theory of gas-pressure each constituent exerts a pressure corresponding to the proportion of that gas present. In atmospheric air, therefore, the oxygen, being present to the extent of 20 per cent., exerts a pressure of j* of an atmosphere or £X 760 = 152 mms. Hg. When we speak of one atmosphere of gas pressure, therefore, we mean a pressure equivalent to 760 mms. Hg, and in any given mixture the pressure exerted by any constituent may be expressed in percentages or fractions of an atmosphere, or in the equivalent height of the mercury column which it will support. Absorption of Gases in Liquids. — When a gas is brought into contact with a liquid with which it does not react chemically a certain number of the moving gaseous molecules penetrate the liquid and become dissolved. Some of these dissolved molecules •escape from the water from time to time, again becoming gaseous. It is evident, however, that if a liquid, water, is brought into contact with a gas under definite pressure, — that is, containing a definite number of molecules to a unit volume, — an equilibrium will be established. As many molecules will penetrate the liquid in a given time as escape from it, and the liquid will hold a definite number of the gas molecules in solution; it will be saturated for that pressure of gas. If the pressure of the gas is increased, how- ever, an equilibrium will be established at a higher level and more molecules of gas will be dissolved in the liquid. Experiments have shown, in accordance with this mechanical conception, that the 652 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. amount of a given gas dissolved by a given liquid varies, the temper- ature remaining the same, directly with the pressure, — that is, it in- creases and decreases proportionally with the rise and fall of the gas pressure. This is the law of Henry. On the other hand, the amount of gas dissolved by a liquid varies inversely with the temperature. It follows, also, from the same mechanical views that in a mixture of gases each gas is dissolved in proportion to the pressure that it exerts, and not in proportion to the pressure of the mixture. Air consists, in round numbers, of 4 parts of N and 1 part of 0. Consequently, when a volume of water is exposed to the air the oxygen is dissolved according to its "partial pressure," — that is, under a pressure of -g- of an atmosphere (152 nuns. Hg). The water will contain only ^ as much oxygen as it would if exposed to a full atmosphere of oxygen — that is, to pure oxygen. And, on the other hand, if water has been saturated with oxygen at one atmosphere (760 nuns.) of pressure and is then exposed to air, four-fifths of the dissolved oxygen will be given off, since the pressure of the surrounding oxygen has been diminished that much. Ab- sorption coefficient. By this term is meant the number that ex- presses the proportion of gas dissolved in a unit volume of the liquid under one atmosphere of pressure. The absorption coefficient will vary, of course, with the temperature. The gases that interest us in this connection are oxygen, nitrogen, and carbon dioxid. The absorption coefficients of these gases for the blood at the tempera- ture of the body are as follows: O, 0.0262: N, 0.0130; C02, 0.5283.* Thai is, 1 c.c. of blood at body temperature dissolves 0.0262 of 1 c.c. of oxygen if exposed to an atmosphere of pure oxygen, and so on. The solubility of the C02 is therefore twenty times as great as that of oxygen. Accepting these figures, we may calculate how much of 1 1 1 esc three gases can be held in the arterial blood in physical solution, provided we know the pressure of the gases in the alveoli of the lungs. The composition of the alveolar air will be discussed 'farther on, but we may assume at present that i1 contains 80 per cent, of nitrogen, 15 per cent, of oxygen, and 5 per cent, of carbon dioxid. In 100 C.C. of blood, therefore, the following amounts of these gases should be held in solution: Nitrogen 100X0.013 X 0.80 l .01 c.c. ■ i gen loo x 0.0202 x 0.15 - 0.393 " Carbon dioxid LOO X 0.5283 X 0.05 2.64 " As will be seen from I he analyses given above of I he actual amounts of Hi- obtained from the blood, the nitrogen alone is present in quantities corresponding to what would be expected if it is held in simple physical solution. * As given by Bohr, the absorption coefficients <>f these three gases at in C. are a folio 0 jrgen 0.0231; nitrogen, 0.0118; carbon dioxid, 0.530. CHANGES IN AIR AND BLOOD IN RESPIRATION. 653 The Tension or Pressure of Gases in Solution or Combi- nation.— When a gas is held in solution the equilibrium is de- stroyed if the pressure of this gas in the surrounding medium or atmosphere is changed. If this pressure is increased the liquid takes up more of the gas, and an equilibrium is established at a higher level. If the pressure is decreased the liquid gives off some of the gas. That pressure of the gas in the surrounding atmosphere at which equilibrium is established measures the tension of the gas in the liquid at that time. Thus, when a bowl of water is exposed to the air the tension of the oxygen in solution is 152 mms. Hg; that of the nitrogen is 608 mms. Hg. If the same water is exposed to pure oxygen the tension of the oxygen in solution is equal to 760 mms. Hg, while that of the nitrogen sinks to zero if the gas that is given off from the water is removed. With compounds such as oxyhemoglobin the tension under which the oxygen is held is measured by the pressure of the gas in the sur- rounding atmosphere at which the compound neither takes up nor gives off oxygen. If, therefore, it is necessary to determine the tension of any gas held in solution or in dissociable combination it is sufficient to determine the percentage of that gas in the surrounding atmosphere and thus ascertain the partial pressure that it exerts. If the atmosphere contains 5 per cent, of a given gas the partial pressure exerted by it is equal to 38 mms. Hg (760 X 0.05), and this figure expresses the tension under which the gas is held in solution or combination in a liquid exposed to such an atmosphere. As regards the tension of the gases in arterial and venous blood, this procedure is, of course, not possible, since the blood is sur- rounded, not by an atmosphere whose composition can be analyzed, but by the liquids of the body, the lymph and cell juices. To determine the tension of the gases in the blood it is necessary to remove the blood from the vessels and bring it into contact with an atmosphere containing a known quantity of O, C02, or N, according to the gas to be measured. By trial an atmosphere can be obtained in which this gas is contained in amounts such that there is no marked increase or decrease in quantity after standing in diffusion relations with the blood. The percentage of the gas in the atmos- phere chosen will measure the tension of that gas in the blood. An instrument which has been much used for such determinations is represented diagrammatically in Fig. 265. It is known as an aero- tonometer (Pfliiger). It consists of a tube (A) which can be con- nected through b directly with the blood-vessels. This tube A is surrounded by a jacket (O) containing warm water, so that the blood may be kept at the body temperature during the experiment. A is first completely filled with mercury from the bulb M to drive out the air. An atmosphere of known composition is then sucked into A by dropping the bulb. Blood is allowed to flow into A through 654 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. the stopcock b and to trickle down the sides of the tube. Diffusion relations are set up between the blood and the known atmosphere, and after equilibrium has been established the gas is driven out through a into a convenient receiver and analyzed. If two aero- tonometers are used, one containing the gas at somewhat higher pressure than that expected, and the other at a somewhat lower pressure, an average result is ob- tained which expresses with suffi- cient accuracy the pressure of the given gas in the blood. It is important not to confuse the tension at which a gas is held in a liquid with the volume of the gas. Thus, blood exposed to the air contains its oxygen under a tension of 152 mms. Hg, but the amount of oxygen is equal to 20 volumes per cent. Water exposed to the air contains its oxygen un- der the same tension, but the amount of gas in solution is' less than 1 volume per cent. Tensions of gases in liquids are expressed either in percentages of an atmos- phere or in millimeters of mercury. Thus, the tension of oxygen in ar- terial blood is found to be equal to about 13 per cent, of an atmos- phere or 100 mms. Hg. (760 X 0. 1 3) . The Condition and Signifi- cance of the Nitrogen. — We may accept the view that the nitrogen of the blood is held in physical solution. The amount present corresponds willi this view, and, moreover, it is found thai the quantity varies directly with the pressure in accordance with the law given above. If an animal I permitted to breathe an atmosphere of oxygen and hydrogen the nitrogen di appears from the blood, and when ordinary air is breathed the nitrogen contents of the arterial and venous bloods exhibit no constant difference in quantity. Ii seems certain, there fore, thai i lie nitrogen plays no direct role in the physiological pro- h i absorbed by the blood in proportion to its partial 1'iK- 200. — Diagram to show the principle of the aerotonometer: A, The tube containing a known mixture of eases, O, CO3, N; C, the outside jacket for maintaining a con tan! hoily tem- perature. When topcook 6 1 open the blood trickles down the side 01 I and enters into 'lilTu ion relations with the contained gs e , Aitei equilibrium is reached "lie topcock u i closed and a i opened. By meane of the mer- cury liulli the ga e can then l"- forced out of .1 into a uiiaUe receiver lor anal'. 1 CHANGES IN AIR AND BLOOD IN RESPIRATION. 655 pressure in the alveoli of the lungs and circulates in the blood in small amounts without exerting any immediate influence upon the tissues. Condition of Oxygen in the Blood. — That the oxygen is not held in the blood merely in solution is indicated, in the first place, by the large quantity present and, in the second place, by the fact that this quantity does not vary directly with the pressure in the surrounding medium. It is definitely known that by far the largest portion of the oxygen is held in chemical combination with the hemoglobin of the red corpuscles, while a much smaller portion, varying with the pressure, is held in solution in the plasma. The compound oxyhemoglobin possesses the important property that when the pressure of oxygen in the surrounding medium falls suffi- ciently it begins to dissociate and free oxygen is given off. The proc- 0 10 ^b 30 40 SO 60 70 80 90 100 110 110 130 140 150 160 Fig. 266. — Curves of dissociation of the oxyhemoglobin at different pressures of oxygen. Five curves are shown to indicate that the dissociation of the oxyhemoglobin is greatly influenced by the presence of COo. The figures along the ordinates (10 to 100) indicate percentages of saturation of the hemoglobin with oxygen, while the figures along the abscissa (0 to 160) indicate different pressures of oxygen. The curve marked 5 mm. CO2 shows the amount of combination of oxygen and hemoglobin when the CO* is absent or present only in traces. In this curve at a pressure of 30 mms. of oxygen it will be seen that the hemoglobin is 80 per cent, saturated with oxygen, while with a pressure of 40 mms. of COo, which approximates that in the body, the hemoglobin at the same pressure of oxygen is only 50 per cent, saturated. (After Bohr.) ess of dissociation is facilitated also by increase of temperature, provided, of course, that it does not rise to the point of coagulating the hemoglobin. The amount of dissociation that takes place under different pressures of oxygen in the surrounding medium has been studied both for solutions of pure hemoglobin * and for defibrinated blood. t It would seem from recent work that the compound between oxygen and hemoglobin is more easily dissociated when the hemoglobin is in its natural condition in the corpuscles than when it - * Hiifner, " Archiv f. Physiologie," suppl. volume, 1901, p. 213. t Loewy, "Archiv f. Physiologie," 1904, p. 245. 656 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATIOX. has been crystallized out and obtained in pure solutions. The re- sults that have been obtained from experiments upon defibrinated blood probably represent, therefore, more nearly the conditions of dissociation in the body. The results obtained by Bohr are indicated in the curve of dissociation shown in Fig. 266, obtained from experiments on dog's blood. At a pressure of oxygen of 152 mms. — that is. when exposed to ordinary air — the hemoglobin is nearly or completely saturated with oxygen. If the oxygen pressure is increased, — if, for instance, the blood is exposed to pure oxygen (pressure, 760 mms.), — no more oxygen is combined chemically by the hemoglobin. Additional oxygen will be taken up by the blood, but only in so far as it can pass into solution in the blood-plasma. Oxygen thus dissolved in the blood-plasma obeys the physical law of solution, and will be at once given off when the oxygen pressure of the surrounding medium is lowered. If the pressure of oxygen falls below that of the air (152 mms.) the chemi- cally combined oxyhemoglobin begins to dissociate slowly at first, but as the pressure falls below 70 mms. the dissociation becomes much more rapid, and the oxygen thus liberated from chemical combination is from a quantitative standpoint much more impor- tant than that freed from solution in the plasma. This, in fact, is the process that takes place as the blood circulates through the tissues. The arterial blood enters the capillaries with its hemo- globin nearly saturated with oxygen, — about 19 c.c. to each 100 c.c. of blood. After it leaves the capillaries the venous blood contains only about 12 volumes of oxygen to each 100 c.c. of blood. In the passage of the capillaries, which takes only about one second, the blood loses, therefore, about 'A5 per cent, or more of its oxygen. The physical theory of respiration furnishes data to show that this In-- is due to a dissociation of the oxyhemoglobin, owing to the lad that in passing through the capillaries the blood is brought into exchange with a Burrounding medium lymph, cell liquid — in which the oxygen pressure is very low. A fact of subsidiary importance in this connection is shown in the curves reproduced in Fig. 266. It will be noted in this figure that the dissociation of the oxyhemoglobin is facilitated by an increase in the pressure of the carbon dioxid. In the tissues where the oxyhemoglobin is broken up there is always a certain tension of carbon dioxid, a pressure which lies somewhere between 40 and SO nuns, of mercury, and the presence of t his gas in this proportion probably helps the dissociation of the oxyhemoglobin to the extent .shown by the curve- in this figure. Condition of the Carbon Dioxid in the Blood.— The condition in which the carbon dioxid is held in the blood is not entirely understood. It has long been recognized that a certain small CHANGES IN AIR AND BLOOD IN RESPIRATION. 657 percentage is held in simple physical solution in the plasma and in the corpuscles, and that a certain additional amount, much larger than the preceding, is chemically combined with the alkali of the blood as a carbonate, most probably as a bicarbonate (HNaC03). It has been suggested, in fact, that the carbon dioxid of the venous blood is carried chiefly as a bicarbonate and that in passing through the lungs this compound gives off some of its carbon dioxid and is converted into a carbonate, according to the equation 2HNaC03 = Na2C03 + C02 + H20. It is known, however, that an aqueous solution of bicarbonate of soda does not give off carbon dioxid when exposed to low pressures of the gas with anything like the facility shown by blood. Con- sequently it was further assumed that the proteins of blood, acting like weak acids, tend to combine with the alkali and that this additional factor suffices to explain the relative ease with which the bicarbonate as it exists in blood breaks up into carbon dioxid and carbonate. This theory has not proved to be com- pletely satisfactory. Other facts tend to show that the available alkali of the blood exists as bicarbonate in the arterial as well as in the venous blood, and, indeed, the total amount of the alkali in the blood in combination as carbonate or phosphate is not sufficient to account for the quantity of carbon dioxid normally present. In recent years an additional possibility has been suggested by the discovery (Bohr) that carbon dioxid forms a dissociable compound with hemoglobin (p. 417), and the probability that a similar compound may be formed with the proteins of the plasma. Accepting this suggestion it would seem that the carbon dioxid exists in the blood in three forms. The amounts present in each form is estimated by Loewy* as follows: In each 100 c.c. of arterial blood, containing normally 40 volume per cent, of carbon dioxid, there is Physically absorbed in plasma and corpuscles 1.9 per cent. Held as sodium bicarbonate { g Jgg^; ] ] ™ } ■ . 18.8 << « Held in organic combination { £ ^ck ; ; ™ } . . 19.3 " « When serum or plasma is exposed to a vacuum at body tem- perature only a portion of the carbon dioxid is given off; to obtain the balance it is necessary to add acid to the liquid. This latter portion, liberated only by a stronger acid, is spoken of as the "fixed carbon dioxid." If instead of exposing serum or plasma to a vacuum one uses full blood, that is. plasma or serum plus corpuscles, all the carbon dioxid may be obtained without the necessity of adding acid. This fact has been explained on * Loewy, " Handbuch d. Biochemie," 1908, IV1. 42 658 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. the supposition that the hemoglobin under these conditions plays the part of an acid in breaking up the compound in which the carbon dioxid is firmly held. Presumably this fixed carbon dioxid is the portion which in the above classification is repre- sented as bicarbonate. Since the portion that is held in organic combination is apparently more easily dissociated, it seems likely that it furnishes the main compound which is physio- logically useful in providing a means for the transportation of carbon dioxid from the tissues, where it is formed, to the lungs, where it is eliminated. The Physical Theory of Respiration. — The physical theory of respiration assumes that the gaseous exchange in the lungs and in the tissues takes place in accordance with the physical laws of diffusion of gases. If a permeable membrane separates two vol- umes of any gas, or two solutions of any gas at different pressures, the molecules of the gas will pass through the membrane in both directions until the pressure is equal on both sides. As the excess of movement is from the point of higher pressure to the point of lower pressure, attention is paid only to this side of the process, and we say that the gas diffuses from a point of high tension to one of lower tension. After equilibrium is established and the pressure is the same on both sides we must imagine that the diffusion is equal in both directions, and the condition is the same as though there were no further diffusion. In order for this theory to hold for the exchange in the body it must be shown that the physical conditions are such as it demands. Numerous experi- ments have been made, therefore, to determine the actual pressure of the oxygen and carbon dioxid in the venous blood as com- pared with the pressures of the same gases in the alveolar air, and the pressures in the arterial blood as compared with those in the tissues. Although the actual figures obtained have varied some- what with the method used, the species or condition of the animal, yet, on the whole, the results tend to support the physical theory. The Gaseous Exchange in the Lungs. — It is impossible to determine the exact composition of the alveolar air. The expired air ran, of course, be collected and analyzed, but obviously this is a mixture of the air in the bronchi and the alveoli, and consequently has more oxygen and less carbon dioxid than the air in the alveoli. The probable composition of the alveolar air has been calculated by Zuntz and Loewy for normal quiet breathing in the following way: The capacity of the bronchial tree is 140 c.c, and this air may be considered as similar in composition to atmospheric air, that is, the inspired air. A normal expiration contains 500 c.c.j hence the alveolar air constitutes only 360 c.c. or !* of the entire amount. If the expired air contains 1.38 per cent, of C02, then the alveolar air musl contain 4.36h-^-£ or 0 per cent, of carbon dioxid. CHANGES IN AIR AND BLOOD IN RESPIRATION. 659 Or, to put the mode of calculation in a more general form, the amount of oxygen in the expired air is equal to the amount of oxygen in the true alveolar portion of the expired air plus the amount of oxygen in the "dead space, " namely, the trachea and bronchi. Let A equal the volume of expired air, e the percentage of oxygen in the expired air, a the volume of air in the dead space, and i the percentage of oxygen in this air or what is the same thing in the inspired air. According to the above statement we have the fol- lowing equation, Ae = ai -f- (A — a) ,x, in which x represents the unknown percentage of oxygen in the alveolar air. We have, therefore, x = . - . In ordinary breathing these values are as follows: A — 500 c.c, a = 140 c.c, e = 16.02 per cent., and i = 20.96 per cent. Substituting these values, x will be found equal to 14.1 per cent. Reckoned in millimeters of mercury this would be equal to (760 X 0.141) 107.2 mm. In order, however, to ascertain the' true pressure exerted by the oxygen allowance must be made for the baro- metric pressure and for the tension of the aqueous vapor. In the depths of the lungs the air is saturated with water vapor and the tension of this vapor at the body temperature may be valued at 46.6 mms. Hg. If we suppose further that the observation was made at a barometric pressure of 750 mms., then the pressure of the oxygen in the alveoli would be (750 — 46.6X0.141) 99+ mms. Hg. Actual observations made by these authors upon human beings in whom the expired air was analyzed indicate that the composition of the alveolar air may vary between the following limits: Oxygen between 11 and 17 per cent, of an atmosphere; carbon dioxid be- tween 3.7 and 5.5 per cent, of an atmosphere. Loewy and von Schrotter have determined also the average ten- sion of these gases in the blood of man. Their method * consisted in blocking off one lung or one lobe of a lung by a metal catheter inserted through the trachea. After the lapse of half an hour or so the gases in this occluded portion had reached an equilibrium by interchange with the venous blood which represented the tension actually existing in the circulating venous blood. A portion of this air was then withdrawn by means of a suitable device and was analyzed. Their average result was that in the venous blood the oxygen exists under a tension of 5.3 per cent, of an atmosphere (710 X .053 — 37.6 mms. Hg), and the C02 under a tension of 6 per cent. (42.6 mms. Hg). The physical relations of pressure between the alveolar air and the gases in the venous blood may be represented as follows : Oxygen. Carbon Dioxid. Alveolar air 100 mms. 35 to 40 mms. Membrane + Y I Venous blood . . . 37.6 mms. 42.6 mms. Diffusion must take place, therefore, in the direction indicated by the arrows. As the oxygen passes through into the blood it is combined with the hemoglobin and it is estimated that the arterial blood as it flows awa}^ from the lungs is nearly saturated with * Loewy and von Schrotter, "Zeitschrift fur experimentelle Pathologie und Therapie," 1, 197, 1905. See also Loewy, "Handbuch der Biochemie," IV1, 1908. 660 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. oxygen, lacking perhaps only 1 volume per cent, of being completely saturated (Pfliiger). That is, if the normal arterial blood contains 19 c.c. of oxygen for each 100 c.c. of blood, it is probable that one more cubic centimeter might be combined by the hemoglobin if exposed fully to the air or oxygen. The difference in tension between the carbon dioxid on the two sides of the membrane is not so great as in the case of the oxygen, but owing to the more rapid diffusion of this gas it is probable that this difference suffices to explain the exchange. In this matter one must bear in mind also the very large expanse of surface offered by the lungs and the very complete subdivision of the mass of blood in the capillaries. Thus, following a calculation made by Zuntz, the surface of the human lungs may be estimated at 90 sq.ms. or 900,000 sq.cms. If we assume that 300 c.c. of carbon dioxid (500 X 0.04 X 15) are given off from the blood in a minute this would indicate a diffusion through each square centimeter of only 0.0003 c.c. (to3o°oVo)- This same idea is expanded by Loewy as follows: The surface of the lungs exposed to the air may be reckoned at 90 square meters, and the thick- ness hi membrane mtervening between this air and the blood in the capillaries may be estimated at 0.004 of a millimeter. Under these conditions as much as 6083 c.c. of oxygen might diffuse into the blood in a minute. As a matter of fact only about 250 to 300 c.c. of oxygen are really absorbed per minute is quiet breathing, and not more than ten times this amount in the violent respiration following excessive muscular exercise. It would seem, therefore, that diffusion should suffice to supply the oxygen actually needed. This reasoning applies a fortiori to the carbon dioxid, since the velocity of diffusion of this gas through a moist membrane is much (25 times) greater. If the tension of the ('<). in the blood were only 0.03 mm. higher than that in the alveoli, the known exchange might be explained by diffusion. Exchange of Gases in the Tissues.— The arterial blood passes to the tissues nearly saturated with oxygen so far as the hemo- globin is concerned, and this oxygen is held under a tension equivalent probably to at least 100 mms. Hg. The carbon dioxid is less in quantity than on entering the lungs and exists under a .smaller pressure, which may be assumed to be the same as that of the carbon dioxid in the alveoli of the lungs — namely, 5 per cent, of an atmosphere (35 mms.). In the systemic capil- laries the blood comes into diffusion relations with the tissues, and direct examination of the latter shows thai the oxygen in them exists under a very small pressure, practically zero pres- sure, while the ( ,( )2 is present under a tension (Strassburg) of 7 to 9 per cent. The high tension of the C02 is explained by the fad that it is being formed in the tissues constantly as a resull of their metabolism, while the low tension of the oxygen is n of ;i different ♦ For discussion and literature, sec Meltzer, "Archiv I'. Physiologic, " 1892, p. 340; also "New York Medical Journal," January 18, 1890. Lewan- dowsky, "Archiv f. Physiologic, " 1896, pp. 195 and 183. t Einthoven, "Quarterly Journal of Exp. Physiology," 1908, L, 243: i:. earche of the Physiological Laboratory of the I University of Leyden/ VII., 1908. INNERVATION OF THE RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 669 character was produced, indicating the existence of a separate set of fibers brought into action by the diminution in volume of the lungs. In quiet respirations the expiration consists in merely a passive return to what may be called the neutral or normal volume of the lungs, and in this movement it is probable that the inspiratory fibers are not affected, being stimulated only by an active expiration. We may assume, therefore, with Gad that the normal rate of respirations is maintained + 1 1 1 1-j.l 1 (+ -f+t -i* - '; Fig. 271. — To show the electrical changes in the different fibers of the vagus nerve caused by the respirations and the heart beats: V, The electrovagogram, the large waves are electrical oscillations synchronous with the respiratory movements. The smaller ones are electrical changes synchronous with the heart beats; P, Mechanical record of the respiratory movements, ascent of curve, inspiration; C, Mechanical record of pulse beats. (From Einthoven.) by the action of the inhibitory fibers alone. Each inspiration is cut short by the mechanical stimulation of these fibers, but on the collapse of the lungs the new inspiration is due to a normal discharge from the inspiratory center. Loewy*'has shown by an ingenious experiment that the expansion of the lungs is the factor that actually stimulates the sensory fibers and quickens the respiratory rate, as follows : An animal was made to breathe pure oxygen for a while to displace the nitrogen in the alveoli. The chest on one side — say, the right side — was then opened with the result that the lung collapsed, and, owing to the rapid absorption of the oxygen, soon became practically solid. The respirations (rabbit) showed their normal rate— 66. The vagus nerve on the left side was then cut and immediately the respirations took on the character usually shown when both vagi are severed, — respirations = 34. Next the collapsed right lung was expanded by artificial respiration, with the result that the respiratory rate at once returned to normal. Respiratory Reflexes from the Larynx, Pharynx, and Nose. — The mucous membrane of the larynx receives its sensory fibers from the superior laryngeal nerve. When this nerve is stimulated artificially the respirations are always inhibited ; the chest comes to rest in the position of passive expiration. The same effect may be * "Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie," 42, 273 670 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. obtained from the sensory fibers of the glossopharyngeal supplying the pharynx, and indeed a temporary inhibition of respirations occurs through this nerve during every act of swallowing. The sensory fibers of the nasal mucous membrane (trigeminal) cause a similar reflex inhibition when stimulated by injurious or so called irrespirable gases, such as HC1, CI, NH3, S02, etc. We may regard this inhibitory influence exerted by the sensory fibers distributed along the air passages as a protective reflex which guards the lungs automatically from injurious gases. This protective action is made more evident by the fact that, together with the cessation of respirations, the glottis is reflexly closed by contraction of the ad- ductor muscles and, if the stimulation is strong, even the bronchial: musculature may be contracted, so that in every way the passage to the alveoli is made more difficult. The reflex is, of course, more or less temporary, but it possesses the great advantage of being automatic, and may enable the animal or individual to escape unharmed from a dangerous locality before the increasing irritabil- ity of the respirator}- center breaks through the inhibition. In special cases the inhibition may last for an unusually long time. Thus, Fredericq states that in aquatic birds water allowed to flow over the beak so as to penetrate slightly into -the nostrils brings about an inhibition of respirations for many minutes. There would seem in this case to be a special adaptation of the reflex to the needs of diving. We know also that irritating gases or foreign bodies of any sort that enter the larynx may lead to a coughing reflex, — that is, to a series of expiratory blasts which have a pur- poseful end in the expulsion of the stimulating object. In this case there is not simply an inhibition of the inspiratory movements, but a reflex excitation of a peculiar type of expiratory movements. The Voluntary Control of the Respiratory Movements. — We can control the respiratory movements within wide limits, make forced or feeble inspirations or expirations, accelerate the rhythm, or completely inhibit the respirations in any phase. If, however, the "breath is held," — that is, if the respiratory movements are inhibited and the glottis is closed, the increasing ' irritability of the respiratory center eventually breaks through the voluntary inhibition. How far this voluntary control is based upon direct connections between the cerebrum and the respiratory center and how far it depends upon voluntary paths to the separate spinal nuclei of the muscles involved cannot be discussed profitably. The Nature of the Respiratory Center. — The respiratory center located in the medulla oblongata might with more propriety be designated as the inspiratory center. Our normal respirations throughout life consist of an active inspiration and a passive expiration. It is the co-ordinated activity of the inspiratory INNERVATION OF THE RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 671 muscles that is characteristic of the respiratory movements. The expiratory muscles come into action only occasionally and under special conditions. It is, in reality, incorrect to speak of the normal respirations as consisting of alternate inspiratory and expiratory movements; as a matter of fact, thej^ consist of rhythmical in- spiratory movements alone. So also when we describe the respira- tory center as essentially automatic we refer only to the action on the inspiratory muscles, since a series of active inspiratory move- ments is the essential feature of respiration. Under certain con- ditions, however, we do have rhythmical expiratory movements, active expirations. Such movements may occur independently of the respirations proper, as in coughing and laughing, or in the straining movements of defecation, micturition, and parturition; or they may occur as an integral part of the respirations, as in the forced movements of dyspnea. Under the conditions of partial suffocation, for instance, as the blood becomes more and more venous the respirations increase in force and active expirations appear. It becomes a question, therefore, as to the existence of what might be called an expiratory center, a group of nerve cells controlling the co-ordinated activity of the expiratory muscles. The mere fact that in dyspnea we have a rhythmical and co-ordi- nated activity of these muscles seems to imply the existence of such a center, but there is no definite experimental knowledge as to its location. Assuming that there is such a center, it may be believed that it exists in the medulla, since after section below the medulla there is no evidence of the occurrence of rhythmical expiratory movements even in extreme conditions of venosity of the blood. The expiratory center may or may not be located in the same region as the inspiratory center, but the following general char- acteristics may be assigned to it : In the first place, it is not auto- matic ; at least not under normal conditions. In the second place, its activity must be dependent in some way upon that of the in- spiratory center. Even our most violent respiratory movements show an orderly sequence of inspiration and expiration, — and we may believe that the action of the expiratory center is conditioned by the previous discharge of the inspiratory center, just as in the heart the beat of the ventricle depends upon the previous systole of the auricle. That an active expiration is not caused reflexly by the mechanical expansion of the lungs seems to be demonstrated by the fact that the most forcible voluntary inspiration is followed by a passive, not an active expiration. Until our knowledge is extended by further experimental work we may consider the ex- piratory center as a group of cells connected by definite paths with the expiratory muscles and capable of being stimulated in one of at least four general ways: (1) In special reflexes, such as coughing. 672 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. (2) By voluntary control from the cerebrum, as in straining. (3) By stimulation through afferent fibers from the skin, especially the pain fibers. (4) By the action of an increased venosity of the blood. Under the latter two conditions it is possible that the irritability of the center is so increased that it becomes responsive to the in- fluence of the inspiratory center. The relations of the inspiratory and expiratory centers under the various conditions of artificial stimulation are very complex, and although it is possible to rep- resent these relations more or less completely by schemata of some sort it does not seem advisable at present to seriously consider such hypotheses. The Accessory Respiratory Centers of the Midbrain. — Several observers have called attention to the existence of a possible accessory respiratory center in the midbrain at the level of the posterior colliculus. Martin and Booker found that stimulations in this region caused a marked increase in the rate of inspiratory movements and finally a standstill in inspiration, — that is, a complete tetanic contraction of the inspiratory muscles lasting during the stimulation.* Lewandowsky f has shown that section of the brain stem at or below the inferior colliculi causes an alteration in the respiratory rhythm similar to that following section of both vagi. After cutting through the inferior colliculi further sections more posteriorly do not add to the effect. He considers that there is an automatic inhibitory center in the midbrain which influences continually the automatic activity of the medullary center. The Nature of the Automatic Stimulus to the Respiratory Center. — We have accepted the view that the respiratory (inspira- tory) center is essentially automatic, although very sensitive to reflex stimulation. The further question arises as to the nature of the automatic stimulus. Inasmuch as the activity of the center controls the gaseous exchanges of the blood, it was natural perhaps for physiologists to look to the gases of the blood for the origin of the internal stimulus. Experiments show beyond question that the condition of the gases in the blood has a direct and marked influence upon the activity of the center. If for any reason the blood supplying the center becomes more venous, the respirations are increased in force or rate or both, and indeed the activity of the center is in a general way increased in proportion to the venosity of the blood. < )n the other hand, if the blood supplying the center is more arterialized than normal, by active ventilation of the lungs, for instance, the center nets more feebly or may fail to act altogether, giving the condition known as apnea. These facts may be accepted ■ completely demonstrated, but they do not go far enough. When peak of the arterial Mood being more venous than normal we mean thai it contains less oxygen and more carbon dioxid than normal arterial blood. Which of these conditions serves to stimulate ♦Martin and Booker, "Journal of Physiology," 1, 370, 1878. f'Archiv f. Physiologic," 1890,489. INNERVATION OF THE RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 673 the center, and which may be regarded as the constant stimulus throughout life ? The three possible views have been defended: (1) That the normal stimulus is a lack of sufficient oxygen (Rosen- thal). When sufficient 0 is supplied the center ceases to act, becomes apneic. (2) That the normal stimulus is the presence of an excess of CO., (Traube). When this excretion is quickly re- moved the center ceases to act, — becomes apneic. (3) It is possible that the two factors may co-operate. The blood that flows through the center may stimulate the cells by virtue of the fact that it does not remove the C02 fast enough and does not supply sufficient oxygen. Much evidence has been collected to show that the action of the respiratory center is increased when the tension of the C02 in the blood is raised without altering that of the oxygen and that a similar result is obtained if the tension of oxygen is greatly diminished without any change in that of the carbon dioxid, so that it must be admitted that a change in either factor, if suffi- ciently great, acts as a stimulus. Experiments, however, have indicated that the accumulation of the C02 is the more efficient stimulus of the two.* Zuntz reports the following interesting experiments, in which the extent of the respiratory movements was measured b}^ the amount of air breathed in a minute. In one series the amount of oxygen in the air breathed was reduced. This change did not affect the quantity of carbon dioxid in the blood. The following results were obtained: Normal air volume breathed per minute = 7,325 to 9,000 c.c. Air with 10 to 11.5 per cent, oxygen " " " " = 8,166 to 9,428 " Air with 8 to 10 per cent, oxygen " " " " = 9,093 to 12,810 " A reduction of one-half of the oxygen in the air breathed had little effect upon the respirations. From our present standpoint, how- ever, the important thing is not the amount of oxygen in the air, but the amount in the blood. Paul Bert's experiments! upon living animals indicate that when the oxygen of the air is reduced by a half the amount of oxygen in the blood is diminished bjr about one-third. Assuming this to be correct, it is evident that a very considerable reduction may be made in the oxygen of the blood without noticeably affecting the respirations. A similar conclusion may be drawn from Haldane's experiments $ with carbon monoxid. He found upon breathing mixtures of this gas that no distinct effects were observable until the blood was about one-third saturated with the gas, — that is, had lost one-third of its oxygen. Zuntz 's ex- * See Zuntz, "Archiv f. Physiol ogie," 1897, 379. See also Friedlander and Herter, " Zeit. f. physiol. Chemie," 2, 99, and 3, 19. t Bert, " La pression barometrique," 1878, 691. % Haldane, "Journal of Physiology," 18, 442, 1895. 43 674 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATIOX. periments, in which the C02 in the air breathed was increased, while the oxygen remained normal, gave quite different results, as follows : Normal air volume breathed per minute, 7,433 c.c. Air of 20.2 per cent. O, 0.95 per cent. CO, " " " " 9.060 " Air of IS. 06 per cent. O, 2.97 per cent. C02 " " " " 11,326 " Air of 18.42 per cent. O, 11.5 per cent. CO,... ■ " " " " 32,464 " These and similar results show that small differences in the amount of the carbon dioxid in the blood have a distinct effect upon the activity of the respirator}' center. Under normal con- ditions the respiratory center receives blood containing 19 to 20 volumes per cent, of oxygen, while the venous blood flowing away from the center still holds 10 to 12 per cent. Considering the small effect of lowering this oxygen supply by one-third, it is difficult to believe that normally the amount of oxygen is so deficient for the normal metabolism as to set up a constant stimulus. The trend of recent work favors rather the view that the normal stimulus to the respiratory center is the carbon dioxid. When this substance is present above a certain amount or tension it acts as a stimulus and gives rise to the moderate movements of normal inspiration. If the tension of the carbon dioxid is increased its stimulating action becomes stronger and leads to the production of a condition of hyperpnea and dyspnea. On the other hand, if for any reason, such as active ventilation of the lungs, the tension of the carbon dioxid in the blood falls below a certain value, estimated by Zuntz as lying between 19 and 24 rams., no stimu- lation occurs, the center is in a condition of apnea and respiratory movements cease. In reference to the effect of carbon dioxid in augmenting the respiratory movements an interesting dis- tinction has been made by Scott* between the rate and the depth of the respirations. From experiments made upon rabbits he finds that an increase of carbon dioxid in the air breathed causes an increase in both the rate and the depth of the respiratory movements, but if the vagi are first cut then raising the per- centage of carbon dioxid has an effect only upon the depth of the respirations. It would appear, therefore, that so far as an increase of carbon dioxid in the lungs affects the rapidity of breathing, it does so by .'in ad ion of some sort upon the sensory fibers of the vagus which end in the lungs. Under special circumstances other substances may occur in the blood which act as chemical stimuli to the center, for example, the acid products formed during muscular contractions (see p. 680). * Scott, "Journal of Physiology," 37, 301, 1908. INNERVATION OF THE RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 675 The Cause of the First Respiratory Movement. — The mam- malian fetus under normal conditions makes no respiratory move- ments while in utero. After birth and the interruption of the pla- cental circulation the first breath is taken. The cause of this sudden awakening to activity on the part of the respiratory center must be closely connected, if not identical with, the cause of the automatic activity of the center throughout life. Two or perhaps three views have been held regarding its immediate cause: (1) That it is due to the increased venosity of the blood brought about by the interruption of the placental circulation; (2) that it is due to stimulation of the skin by handling, drying, etc.; (3) that it is due to a combination of these causes. Preyer has shown that stimula- tion of the skin of the fetus while in utero and with the placental circulation intact sumcies to cause respiratory movements. Cohn- stein and Zuntz* have shown that interruption of the placental circulation while the fetus is kept bathed in the amniotic liquid also brings about respirations. Since both of these events occur normally at birth, we may believe that each aids in causing the first respira- tion, and indeed it may be necessary at times deliberately to in- crease the stimulation of the skin in order to bring on respiratory movements. If the two causes, stimulation through the nerves and stimulation through the blood, normally co-operate, it may, how- ever, be said that the essential cause, according to the theory adopted in the preceding paragraphs, lies in the greater venosity of the blood, that is, the increased tension of the carbon dioxid follow- ing interruption of the placental circulation. During the intra- uterine period it is evident that the fetal blood is aerated so well by exchange with the maternal blood that it does not act as a stimulus to the fetal respiratory center. The fetus is, physiolog- ically speaking, in a condition of apnea. Since the maternal blood acts upon the respiratory center of the mother, while the fetal blood which exchanges gases with it does not act on its own respira- tory center, it follows that the fetal respiratory center possesses a lower degree of irritability than that of the mother. Dyspnea, Hyperpnea, Apnea. — By the term dyspnea in its widest sense we mean any noticeable increase in the force or rate of the respiratory movements. As said above, such a condition may be caused either by stimulation of sensory nerves, particularly the pain nerves or the sensory fibers of the vagus distributed to the lungs themselves, or by an increased venosity of the blood — that is, by an increase in the C02 or by a marked decrease in the oxygen. Changes of other kinds in the com- position of the blood, some of which are considered in the next chapter, may also stimulate the respiratory center and cause * Cohnstein and Zuntz, "Arch. f. die gesarnmte Physiol.," 42, 342, 1S88. 676 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. dyspnea. The dyspneic movements naturally show many degrees of intensity corresponding with the strength of the stimulus, and sometimes the initial stages are designated as hyperpnea, while the term dyspnea is reserved for the more labored breathing in which the expirations are active and forced. When dyspnea is produced by withholding air (suffocation) the respiratory movements become more and more violent until they take on a convulsive character. This stage is succeeded by one of apparent calm, indicative of exhaustion of the centers. Deep, Fig. 272. — To show the recovery from apnea. The animal (rabbit) had been venti- lated with a bellows and thrown into a condition of apnea shown at the beginning of the record. The respirations returned first as feeble movements which gradually in- creased to the normal. — (Dawson.) long-drawn inspirations follow at intervals and finally cease. The animal lies quietly, with feeble heart beat and dilated pupils, in a condition designated as asphyxia or complete asphyxia. The term apnea means literally a condition of no breathing, and since this condition may occur from several causes some confusion in nomenclature has resulted. In medical literature the term is some- times employed as a synonym for asphyxia or suffocation. In physiological literature it is restricted to a very interesting con- dition which is of great importance with reference to the theories of respiration. This condition is one of cessation of breathing movements due to lack of stimulation of the respiratory center. Jt is brought about by rapid and prolonged ventilation of the lungs. If, for instance, in a rabbit or oilier animal, a tracheal cannula is inserted and connected with a bellows or respiration apparatus, the lungs may be inflated artificially at a rapid rate for any given period of time. If such an experiment is per- formed it will be found that when the blasts are stopped the INNERVATION OF THE RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 677 animal makes no breathing movements at all, sometimes for a considerable interval. When the respirations start again they begin with feeble movements, which gradually increase to the normal amplitude (Fig. 272). One may produce a similar condition upon himself, approximately at least, by a series of rapid, forced inspira- tions. The question of importance is: Why does the respiratory center cease to act? Rosenthal explained the phenomenon in terms of his theory that the normal stimulus to the center results from a lack of oxygen. With vigorous artificial respiration he imagined that the blood takes up more oxygen and thus fails to act upon the center. The apnea is clue to overoxygenation of the blood, and indeed this is the definition he gave to the word.* The numerous researches made upon this condition seem to show very clearly that in the ordinary method used to produce it two factors co-operate, namely a change in the condition of the gases of the blood and a stimulation of sensory fibers in the lungs. Since either one of these factors alone may cause a cessation of breathing, some authors have distinguished two kinds of apnea, apnea vera or chemical apnea, and apnea vagi or inhibitory apnea. Whether or not it is proper to speak of this latter condition as apnea depends altogether upon the definition one gives to the term. If we adhere to the definition suggested above, namely, that apnea is a cessation of breathing, due to lack of stimulation of the respiratory center, then the inhibition of respirations produced by stimulation of the vagi, the so-called apnea vagi, ought not to be included under the term. It is generally statedf that after section of the vagi it is more difficult than in the normal animal to produce apnea by vigorous artificial respiration, so doubtless in this last proce- dure, as usually carried out with a bellows, the rapid stimulation of the inhibitory fibers of the vagus by the expansion of the lungs facilitates the production of a true or chemical apnea de- pendent upon a change in the gases of the blood. That chemical apnea in Rosenthal's sense may exist is shown by the fact that after section of both vagi apnea may still be produced by artificial respiration, and, indeed, several observers! find that after sec- tion of both vagi and of the medulla above the respiratory center the animal may still be made apneic. In such cases it is difficult to see any other cause for the apnea than a change in the gases of the blood. Rosenthal assumed that the apnea is due to an overoxygenation of the blood, but since the vigorous respirations * See Rosenthal, in vol. iv., p. 264, of Hermann's "Handbuch der Physi- ologie." t See Head, "Journal of Physiology," 10, 1, and 279, 1889. % Loewy, "Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologic, " 42, 245, 1888; and Langendorff, "Archiv f. Physiologie, " 1888, p. 286. 678 PHYSIOLOGY OF RESPIRATION. lower especially the contents of the blood in C02 it is probable, as insisted upon by Traube, that this latter factor is the more important. In the preceding paragraphs evidence has been given to show that the normal stimulus to the center is due to the presence of C02, and it follows logically that the more com- plete removal of this gas by ventilation of the lungs should be considered as the chief cause of true apnea. Experimentally this view is well borne out by an old observation of Berns, according to which a condition of apnea in a rabbit may be cut short instantly at any moment by a blast of C02 sent into the lungs, a blast of air having no such effect. This observation is further supported by recent experiments by Mosso* upon men, in which he shows that apnea cannot be produced by inflation with carbon dioxid. This author designates the condition of diminished C02 in the blood as acapnia. According to this terminology, true apnea is due to a condition of acapnia. Much other work has tended to strengthen the general view that a certain tension or pressure of C02 in the blood is neces- sary to stimulate the respiratory center, and that if the C02 is washed out to a certain point by unusual ventilation of the lungs (condition of acapnia), then the respiratory center ceases to give off its rhythmic discharges. There is no desire to breathe and the animal lies quiet in a condition of apnea. Voluntary forced respirations in man maintained for some minutes will produce a similar condition. According to the interesting account given by Haldane and Poultonf an apnea may be produced in this way which will last for 100 to 150 seconds, and before the individual begins to breathe again he may become very blue in the face, owing to the loss of oxygen from the blood. HendersonJ has given experimental evidence to show that a marked diminution in the pressure of the C02 in the blood, brought about by forced respiration, may cause not only a condition of apnea but also a feeble rapid heart-beat, with fall of blood-pressure and the symptoms of surgical shock. It is known that a cessation of respirations maybe brought about in still a third way, namely, by a condition of more or less complete anemia produced by shutting off the blood-supply to the respira- tory center. The lack of activity in this case is probably not a true apnea in the sense of the form given above, since we may suppose t hat under these conditions the tension of the carbon in- creases rat her than decreases. In other words, there is no removal of stimulus, but the cells have lost their irritability, probably * Mohho, "Archives italiennes de biologie," 40, I, L903. t Baldane and Poulton, "Journal of Physiology," :V7, :*«.)<), 1(.>(>8. t Henderson, "American Journal of Physiology," 21, 128, 1908. INNERVATION OF THE RESPIRATORY MOVEMENTS. 679 in consequence of a lack of oxygen, and hence fail to respond to stimulation. Innervation of the Bronchial Musculature. — Numerous investigators, using different methods, have demonstrated that the bronchial musculature is supplied through the vagus with motor and inhibitory fibers, bronchoconstrictor and bronchodilator fibers, as they are usually called.* Stimulation of the constrictors causes a narrowing of the bronchi, and therefore increases the resistance to the inflow and outflow of air. Some observers state that these fibers are nor- mally in a condition of tonic activity (Roy and Brown), but others find little evidence for this belief. An artificial tonus — that is, a condition of maintained activity of the constrictor fibers — may be set up by the action of a number of drugs, such as muscarin, pilocarpin, and physostigmin, which in this case, as in so many other instances of autonomic fibers, are supposed to stimulate the endings of the fibers in the lungs. Their effect is removed by the action of atropin. These fibers are stimulated also during the ex- citatory stages of asphyxia. Reflex stimulation of the constrictors is obtained most readily (Dixon and Brodie) by irritation of the nasal mucous membrane, and it seems probable that in bronchial or spasmodic asthma these fibers are also stimulated reflexly. The normal conditions under which the constrictors and dilators are brought into play can scarcely be stated. Irritating vapors or even C02 lead to a bronchoconstriction and this reflex, as stated on p. 669, may be regarded as protective. When a constriction of the bronchial musculature exists it may be abolished by the paralyzing action of atropin, or temporarily by injections of extracts of lobelia or by the anesthetic effect of inhalations of chloroform or ether. Nicotin also causes a dilatation. * For references to literature, see Dixon and Brodie, "Journal of Physi- ology," 29, 97, 1903. CHAPTER XXXVIII. THE INFLUENCE OF VARIOUS CONDITIONS UPON THE RESPIRATIONS. The Effect of Muscular Work upon the Respiratory Move- ments.— It is a matter of common experience that muscular ex- ercise increases the rate and amplitude of the respiratory move- ments. Roughly speaking, the increase is proportional to the amount of muscular work, and the relationship is evidently a bene- ficial adaptation. The greater the amount of work done, the larger will be the amount of C02 produced and the greater will be the need of oxygen. The adaptation was formerly explained in what seemed to be an entirely satisfactory way by assuming that the increased consumption of O and the greater production of C02 in the muscles resulted in rendering the blood more venous, and consequently the respiratory center was stimulated more strongly, and indeed proportionally to the muscular effort. Geppert and Zuntz,* however, have shown by gas analyses that whatever may be the condition of the venous blood during muscular exercise the arterial blood sent out from the left heart shows no constant change in the quantity or tension of the contained gases. They proved, also, that the effect on the center is not simply a reflex from the nerves in the muscles, since when the hind limbs were made to contract by stimulation the respiratory center was affected in the usual way although all the nerve connections were destroyed. They conclude, therefore, that the respiratory effect of muscular work must be due to certain substances produced in the muscle and given off to the blood. Other experiments (Lehmann) make it probable that these substances are the acid products, lactic acid and acid phosphates, known to be formed in muscle during contraction. Dilute acids injected directly into the veins produce a similar result. The adaptation is a most interesting one, since the products that decrease the irritability of the muscle itself seem to cause an increase in excitability of the group of nerve cells constituting the respiratory center. The Effect of Variations in the Composition of the Air Breathed. Variations in the amount of nitrogen in the inspired * Geppert and Zuntz, "Archiv f. , 251, 1902. MOVEMENTS OF THE ALIMENTARY CANAL. 703 and are again separated (see Fig. 276) . These segmentations may proceed at the rate of thirty per minute for a certain time, and the apparent result is that the material is well mixed with the digestive secretions and is brought thoroughly into contact with the absorp- tive walls. During these rhythmical contractions there is no steady progression of the food; it remains in the same region, although subjected to repeated divisions. From time to time the separated x i^Jt ^ r^ r~> r~< ^ <* ' >0 CD CD CD CD cb k. Fig. 276. — Diagram to show the effect of the rhythmical constricting movements of the small intestine upon the contained food. A string of food (1) is divided suddenly into a series of segments (2) ; each of the latter is again divided and the process is repeated a number of times (3 and 4). Eventually a peristaltic wave sweeps these segments forward a certain distance and gathers them again into a long string, as in (1). The process of segmentation is then repeated as described above. (Cannon.) pieces are caught by an advancing peristaltic wave, moved forward a certain distance, and gathered again into a new mass. In this new location the rhythmical contractions again segment and churn the mass before a new peristaltic wave moves it on. According to this description, the rhythmical movements are local contractions (mainly of the circular muscles) which seem to be due to the local distension caused by the food. They occur rhythmically for a certain period and then cease until a new series is started, and it is obvious that they must play a very important part hi promoting both the digestion and absorption of the food. Mall* has suggested that these rhythmical contractions of the circular coats may also act as a pumping mechanism upon the venous plexuses in the walls and thus aid in driving the blood into the portal system. Similar movements have been observed in the human being. f The curious observation is reported! that during the period of fasting (dog) the whole gastro-intestinal canal, although empty, shows at intervals rhythmical con- tractions of its musculature which may last for twenty to thirty minutes (see p. 762). The Nervous Control of the Intestinal Movements. — There is some evidence to show that the rhythmical contractions of the intestines are muscular in origin (myogenic), while the more co- * Mall, "Johns Hopkins Hospital Reports," 1896, i., 37. f Hertz, loc. cit. % BoldirefT, "Archives des sciences biologiques, " 11,1, 1905. 704 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. ordiriated peristaltic movements depend upon the intrinsic nervous mechanism. The intestine is, however, not dependent for either movement upon its connections with the central nervous system. Like the stomach, it is an automatic organ whose activity is simply regulated through its extrinsic nerves. The small intestine and the greater part of the large intestine receive visceromotor nerve fibers from the vagi and the sympathetic chain. The former, according to most observers, when artifically stimulated cause movements of the intestine, and are therefore regarded as the motor fibers. It seems probable, however, that the vagi carry or may carry in some animals inhibitory fibers as well, and that the motor effects usually obtained upon stimulation are due to the fact that in these nerves the motor fibers predominate. The fibers received from the sympathetic chain, on the other hand, give mainly an inhibitory effect when stimulated, although some motor fibers apparently may take this path. Bechterew and Mislawski * state that the sympathetic fibers for the small intestine emerge from the spinal cord as medullated fibers in the sixth dorsal to the first lumbar spinal nerves, (or lower — Bunch) and pass to the sympathetic chain in the splanchnic nerves and thence to the semilunar plexus. The paths of these fibers through the central nervous system are not known, but there are evidently connections extending to the higher brain centers, since psychical states are known to influence the movements of the intestine, and according to some observers stimulation of portions of the cerebral cortex ma}' produce movements or relaxation of the walls of the small and large intestines. Effect of Various Conditions upon the Intestinal Move- ments.— Experiments have shown that the movements of the in- testines may be evoked in many ways in addition to direct stimu- lation of the extrinsic nerves. Chemical stimuli may be applied directly to the intestinal wall. Mechanical stimulation — pinching, for example, or the introduction of a bolus into the intestinal cavity — may start peristaltic movements. Violent movements may be produced also by shutting off the blood-supply, and again temporarily when the supply is re-established. A condition of dyspnea may also start movements in the intestines or in some inhibit movements which are already in progress, the stimu- lus in this case seeming to act upon the central nervous system and to stimulate both the motor and the inhibitory fibers. Oxygen gas within the bowels tends to suspend the movements of the intes- tine, while C02, CH4, and H2S act as stimuli, increasing the move- ment-;. Organic acids, such as acetic, propionic, formic, and * "Archiv f. Physiologie, " 1889, Buppl. volume. MOVEMENTS OF THE ALIMENTARY CANAL. 705 caprylic, which may be formed normally within, the intestine as the result of bacterial action, act also as strong stimulants. Movements of the Large Intestine. — The opening from the small intestine into the large is controlled both by the ileocecal valve and by a sphincter, the ileocecal or ileocolic sphincter. It is stated that this sphincter is normally in tonus and that its condition of tonus is regulated through the splanchnic nerve (Magnus). The musculature in the large intestine has the same general arrangement as in the small, and the usual view has been that the movements are similar, although more infre- quent, so that the material received from the small intestine is slowly moved along while becoming more and more solid from the absorption of water, until in the form of feces it reaches the sigmoid flexure and rectum. Bayliss and Starling state that their law of intestinal peristalsis holds in this portion of the intestine, — that is, local excitation causes a constriction above and a dilatation below the point stimulated. Cannon,* however, from his studies of the normal movements in cats, as seen by the Roentgen rays, comes to the conclusion that the movements in the large intestine show a marked peculiarity previously overlooked. He divides the large intestine into two parts; in the second, cor- responding roughly to the descending colon the food is moved toward the rectum by peristaltic waves. A number of constrictions may be seen simultaneously within a length of some inches. In the ascending and transverse colon and cecum, on the contrary, the most frequent movement is that of antiperistalsis. The food in this portion of the canal is more or less liquid and its presence sets up running waves of constriction, which, beginning somewhere in the colon, pass toward the ileocecal valve. These waves occur in groups separated by periods of rest. The presence of the ileocecal valve prevents the material from being forced back into the small in- testine. The value of this peculiar reversal of the normal move- ment of the bowels at this particular point would seem to lie in the fact that it delays the passage of the material toward the rectum and by thoroughly mixing it gives increased opportunities for the completion of the processes of digestion and absorption. Hertz estimates that in man the food requires about 2 hours to pass from the ileocecal valve to the hepatic flexure and about 4+ hours to reach the splenic flexure. As the colon becomes filled some of the material penetrates into the descending part, where the normal peristalsis carries it very slowly toward the rectum. The large intestine — particularly the descending colon and rectum — receives its nerve supply from two sources (Fig. 277) : (1) Fibers which leave the spinal cord in the lumbar nerves * Cannon, loc. cit, 45 706 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. (second to fifth in cat), pass to the sympathetic chain, and thence to the inferior mesenteric ganglia, which probably forms the termination of the preganglionic fibers. From this point the path is continued by fibers running in the hypogastric nerves and plexus. Stimulation of these fibers has given different results in the hands of various observers, but the most recent work* indicates that they are inhibitory. (2) Fibers that leave the cord in the sacral nerves (second to fourth), form part of the nervi ententes and enter into the hypogastric plexus. When Sympathetic Trunk I.L umbar I. Lumbar ganglion in, H.L.gangl. m.L. M.L.gangl. JV.L. ' lV.L.gangl. V.L VL (jUHql. VI. L Vl.h.gangl. vn.L. VH li c/angl- 1. Sacral U.S. MS. Plttus Jfypoyos/r/aJS Fin. 277. — Schema to show the innervation of (lie rectum ami internal sphincter of the aim.-, ami the formation of the hypogastric plexus. (After Frankt-Hochwart and Frdhlich.) stimulated these fibers cause contractions of the muscular coats; they may be regarded, therefore, as motor fibers. As in the case of the small intestine and stomach, we may assume that these motor and inhibitory fibers serve for the reflex regulation and adaptation of the movements. Defecation. — The undigested and indigestible parts of the food, together with some of the debris and secretions from the alimentary trad eventually reach the sigmoid flexure and rectum. Authorities <\\i)'cv as io whether the rectum normally * Langley and Anderson, "Journal of Physiology," L8, 67, 1895. Bay- aid Starling, ibid., 26, 107, 1900. Also Wischnewsky, in Hermann's "Jahresberichl der Pbysiologie, " vol, \ii., L905. MOVEMENTS OF THE ALIMENTARY CANAL. 707 contains fecal material or not. Skiagraphic observations made by Hertz* seem, however, to establish the fact that in man, at least, this material accumulates in the rectum in the intervals between defecations. The nearly solid material stimulates by its pressure the sensory nerves of the rectum and produces a distinct sensation and desire to defecate. The fecal material is retained within the rectum by the action of the two sphincter muscles which close the anal opening. One of these muscles, the internal sphincter, is a strong band of the circular layer of involuntary muscle which forms one of the coats of the rectum. When the rectum contains fecal material this muscle is thrown into a condition of tonic contraction until the act of defecation begins, when it is relaxed. The external sphincter ani is com- posed of striated muscle tissue and is under the control of the will to a certain extent. It is supplied by a motor nerve, the Nn. hemorrhoidales inferiores, arising from the N. pudendus and eventually from the sacral spinal nerves. This muscle, therefore, like striated muscle in general, is innervated directly from the spinal cord, but it possesses properties which are to some extent intermediate between those of plain and of striated muscle. For example, it differs from the latter and resembles the former in the fact that it does not atrophy after section of its motor nerve; it is much less sensitive to the paralyzing action of curare than the typical striated muscle, and it is stated that its curve of contraction, when it is stimulated through its nerve, exhibits a long latent period and a slow contraction and relaxa- tion. Both the internal and the external sphincter are normally in tonus and unite in protecting the anal opening. The force of the tonic contraction of the internal is somewhat less (30 to 60 per cent.) than that of the external sphincter. f The innerva- tion and control of the internal sphincter is better understood than that of the external. Like the rest of the rectum, it receives motor fibers from the hypogastric plexus by way of the nervus erigens, and inhibitory fibers from the same plexus by way of the hypogastric nerve. It has been possible to show experimentally that each of these sets of fibers may be acted upon reflexly, for example, by stimulation of the sensory nerves in the sciatic. The reflex takes place in this case through the lower portion of the cord. Both the hypogastric nerve and the N. erigens con- tain also afferent fibers. Stimulation of the central end of the severed N. erigens gives a reflex inhibition through the hypo- gastric nerve, and stimulation of the central stump of the cut hypogastric causes a reflex contraction through the N. erigens. * Hertz, "Guy's Hospital Reports," Gl, 389, 1907. f Consult Frankl-Hochwart and Frohlich, "Archiv f. d. ges. Physiologie, " 81, 420. 708 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. It is even stated that these latter reflexes may be obtained when the lumbosacral cord is destroyed, a fact which if correct would indicate a reflex effected through an outlying ganglion (inf. mesenteric ganglion). The act of defecation as it occurs normally is partly a voluntary and partly an involuntary act. The involuntary act consists in peristaltic contractions of the rectum or, indeed, of the whole colon, together with an inhibi- tion of the sphincters. Whether the inhibition of the sphincters is normally entirely an involuntary reflex cannot be stated definitely. No doubt the sensory stimuli arising from the accu- mulation of fecal material would eventually cause in this way a relaxation of the sphincters, but the act of defecation usually takes place before such a strong necessity arises. It is initiated usualh'' hy a voluntary act and it is possible that in such cases the relaxation of both sphincters may be effected by voluntary inhibition acting upon the spinal centers. The voluntary factor in defecation consists mainly in the contraction of the abdominal muscles. When these latter muscles are contracted and at the same time the diaphragm is prevented from moving upward by the closure of the glottis, the increased abdominal pressure is brought to bear upon the abdominal and pelvic viscera, and aids strongly in pressing the contents of the descending colon and sigmoid flexure into the rectum. The pressure in the abdominal cavity is still further increased if a deep inspiration is first made and then maintained during the contraction of the abdominal muscles. Hertz, on the basis of his skiagraphic observations, insists that simul- taneously with the contraction of the abdominal muscles and the closure of the glottis the diaphragm is also contracted and thus aids in bringing pressure to bear upon the pelvic organs. Although the act of defecation is normally initiated by voluntary effort, it may also be carried out ;is a purely involuntary reflex when the sensory stimulus is sufficiently strong. Goltz* has shown that in dogs in which the spinal cord had been severed in the lower thoracic region defecation was performed normally. In later experiments, in which the entire spinal cord was removed except in the cervical and upper part of the thoracic region, it was found that the animal, after it hud recovered from the operation, had normal movement once or twice a day, indicating that the rectum and lower bowels acted by virtue of their intrinsic mechanism. An interesting result of these experi- ments was the fact that the external sphincter suffered no atrophy, although its motor nerve was destroyed, and that it eventually regained its tonic activity. *"Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 8, L60, L874; 63, 362, L896. MOVEMENTS OF THE ALIMENTARY CANAL. 709 It would seem that the whole act of defecation is, at bottom, an involuntary reflex. The physiological center for the move- ment probably lies in the lumbar cord, and it has sensory and motor connections with the rectum and the muscles of defecation. As stated above, the inhibitory fibers to the internal sphincter pass by way of the hypogastric nerve, the motor fibers through the nervus erigens, and both of these nerves contain afferent fibers which may reflexly excite inhibition or contraction. But this center is probably provided also with intraspinal con- nections with the centers of the cerebrum, through which the act may be controlled by voluntary impulses and by various psychical states; the effect of emotions upon defecation being a matter of common knowledge. In infants the essentially in- voluntary character of the act is well known. Vomiting. — The act of vomiting causes an ejection of the con- tents of the stomach through the esophagus and mouth to the exterior. It was long debated whether the force producing this ejection comes from a strong contraction of the walls of the stom- ach itself or whether it is due mainly to the action of the walls of the abdomen. A forcible spasmodic contraction of the abdominal muscles takes place, as may easily be observed by any one upon himself, and it is now believed that the contraction of these muscles is the principal factor in vomiting. Magendie found that if the stomach was extirpated and a bladder containing water was sub- stituted in its place and connected with the esophagus, injection of an emetic caused a typical vomiting movement with ejection of the contents of the bladder. Gianuzzi showed, on the other hand, that upon a curarized animal vomiting could not be produced by an emetic — because, apparently, the muscles of the abdomen were paralyzed by the curare. There are on record a number of ob- servations which tend to show that the stomach is not passive during the act. On the contrary, it may exhibit contractions, more or less violent in character. According to Openchowski,* the pylorus is closed and the pyloric end of the stomach firmly con- tracted so as to drive the contents toward the dilated cardiac por- tion. Cannon states that in cats the normal peristaltic waves pass over the pyloric portion in the period preceding the vomiting and that finally a strong contraction at the "transverse band" com- pletely shuts off the pyloric portion from the body of the stomach, which at this time is quite relaxed. The act of vomiting is, in fact, a complex reflex movement into which many muscles enter. The following events are described : The vomiting is usually preceded by a sensation of nausea and a reflex flow of saliva into the mouth. These phenomena are succeeded or accompanied by retching move- "Arehiv f. Physiologie, " 1889, p. 552. 710 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. ments. which consist essentially in deep, spasmodic inspirations with a closed glottis. The effect of these movements is to compress the stomach by the descent of the diaphragm, and at the same time to increase decidedly the negative pressure in the thorax, and therefore in the thoracic portion of the esophagus. During one of these retching movements the act of vomiting is effected b}^ a convulsive contraction of the abdominal wall that exerts a sudden additional strong pressure upon the stomach. At the same time the cardiac orifice of the stomach is dilated, probably by an inhibition of the sphincter caused by the rise of pressure in the stomach, and according to the above description the fundic end of the stomach is also dilated, while the pyloric end is in strong contraction. The stomach contents are, therefore, forced violently out of the stomach through the esophagus, the negative pressure in the latter probably assisting in the act. The passage through the esophagus is effected mainly by the force of the contraction of the abdominal muscles; there is no evidence of antiperistaltic move- ments on the part of the esophagus itself. During the ejection of the contents of the stomach the glottis is kept closed by the adductor muscles, and usually the nasal chamber is likewise shut off from the pharynx by the contraction of the posterior pillars of the fauces on the palate and uvula. In violent vomit- ing, however, the vomited material may break through this latter barrier and be ejected partially through the nose. Nervous Mechanism of Vomiting. — That vomiting is a reflex act is abundantly shown by the frequency with which it is produced in consequence of the stimulation of sensory nerves or as the result of injuries to various parts of the central nervous system. After lesions or injuries of the brain vomiting often results. Disagreeable emotions and disturbances of the sense of equilibrium may produce the same result. Irritation of the mucous membrane of various parts of the alimentary canal (as, for example, tickling the back of the pharynx with the finger); disturbances of the urogenital apparatus, the liver, and other visceral organs; artificial stimula- tion of the trunk of the vagus and of other sensory nerves, may all cause vomiting. Under ordinary conditions, however, irritation of the sensory nerves of the gastric mucous membrane is the most common cause of vomiting. This effect may result from the prod- ucts of fermentation in the stomach in cases of indigestion, or may he produced intentionally by local emetics, such as mustard, taken into the stomach. The afferent path in this case is through the sensory fibers of the vagus. The efferent paths of the reflex are found in the motor aerves innervating the muscles concerned in the vomiting, namely, the vagus, the phrenics, and the spinal nerves supplying the abdominal muscles. Whether or not there is a def>- MOVEMENTS OF THE ALIMENTARY CANAL. 711 nite vomiting center in which the afferent impulses are received and through which a co-ordinated series of efferent impulses is sent out to the various muscles has not been satisfactorily deter- mined. It has been shown that the portion of the nervous system through which the reflex is effected lies in the medulla, and it ma3^ be observed that the muscles concerned in the act, outside those of the stomach, are respiratory muscles. Vomiting, in fact, consists essentially in a simultaneous spasmodic contraction of expiratory (abdominal) muscles and inspiratory muscles (diaphragm). It has therefore been suggested that the reflex involves the stimulation of the respiratory center or some part of it. Thumas claims to have located a vomiting center in the medulla in the immediate neighbor- hood of the calamus scriptorius. Further evidence, however, is required upon this point. The act of vomiting may be produced not only as a reflex from various sensory nerves, but may also be caused by direct action upon the medullary centers. The action of apomorphin is most easily explained by supposing that it acts directlv on the nerve centers. Foodstuffs CHAPTER XL. GENERAL CONSIDERATIONS UPON THE COMPOSITION OF THE FOOD AND THE ACTION OF ENZYMES. Foods and Foodstuffs. — The term food when used in a popular sense includes everything that we eat for the purpose of nourishing the body. From this point of view the food of mankind is of a most varied character, comprising a great variety of products of the animal and vegetable kingdoms. Chemical analysis of the animal and vegetable foods shows, however, that they all contain one or more of five or six different classes of substances which are usually designated as the foodstuffs (older names, alimentary or proximate principles) on the belief that they form the useful constituent of our foods. The classification of foodstuffs usually given is as follows: f Water. Inorganic salts. Proteins. Albuminoids, a group of bodies belonging to the general group of proteins, but having in some respects a different nutri- tive value. | Carbohydrates. [ Fats. From the scientific point of view, a foodstuff or food may be defined as a substance absolutely necessary to the normal composition of the body, as in the case of water and salts, or as a substance which can be acted upon by the tissues of the body in such a way as to yield energy (heat, for example) or to furnish material for the pro- duction of living tissue. Moreover, to be a food in the physiological sense the substance must not directly or indirectly affect injuriously the normal nutritive processes of the tissues. The five or' six substances mimed above are all foods in this sense. The water and certain salts of sodium, potassium, calcium, magnesium, iron, and perhaps other elements arc absolutely necessary to maintain the normal composition of the tissue. Complete withdrawal of any one of these eonstituents would cause the death of the organism. Proteins, fits, and carbohydrates, on the other hand, are substances whose molecules have a more or less complex- structure. When eaten and digested they enter the body liquids and are employed either in the synthesis of the more complex living matter, or they undergo various chemical changes, spoken of in general as metab- olism, which result, finally in the breaking up of their complex 712 COMPOSITION OF FOOD AND ACTION OF ENZYMES. 713 molecules into simpler compounds. The chemical changes of metab- olism or nutrition are, in the long run, mainly exothermic, — that is, they are attended by the production of heat* Some of the chem- ical or internal energy that held the complex molecules together assumes the form of heat, or perhaps muscular work, after these molecules are broken down by oxidative changes to simpler, more stable structures, such as water, carbon dioxid, and urea. Proteins, fats, and carbohydrates form materials that the tissue cells are adjusted to act upon after they have undergone certain changes during digestion. Other complex organic compounds containing chemical energy are either injurious to the tissues, or they have a structure such that the tissues cannot act upon them. Such substances cannot be considered as foods in the scientific sense. When, therefore, we desire to know the food value of any animal or vegetable product, we analyze it to determine its composition as regards water, salts, proteins, fats, and carbohydrates. The following table compiled by Munk from the analyses given by Konig * may be taken as an indication of the average composition of the most commonly used foods: COMPOSITION OF FOODS. In 100 Parts. Water. Protein. Fat. 76.7 20.8 1.5 73.7 12.6 12.1 36-60 25-33 7-30 87.7 3.4 3.2 89.7 2.0 3.1 13.3 10.2 0.9 35.6 7.1 0.2 13.7 11.5 2.1 42.3 6.1 0.4 13.1 7.0 0.9 13.1 9.9 4.6 10.1 9.0 0.3 12-15 23-26 li-2 75.5 2.0 0.2 87.1 1.0 0.2 90 2-3 0.5 73-91 4-8 0.5 84 0.5 Carbohydrate Meat Eggs Cheese Cows' milk Human milk Wheat flour Wheat bread Rye flour Rye bread Rice Corn Macaroni Peas, beans, lentils Potatoes Carrots Cabbages Mushrooms Fruit An examination of this table shows that the animal foods, par- ticularly the meats, are characterized by their small percentage in carbohydrate and by a relatively large amount of protein or of protein and fat. With regard to the last two foodstuffs, meats differ *See Konig, "Die menschlichen Nahrungs und Genussmittel ": and Atwater and Bryant, "The Chemical Composition of American Food Mate- rials," Bulletin 28, United States Department of Agriculture, 1899. •14 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AXD SECRETION. very much among themselves. Some idea of the limits of variation may be obtained from the following table, taken chiefly from Konig's analyses: • Water. Protein. Fat. Carbohydrate. Ash. Beef, moderately fat .... Veal, fat Mutton, moderately fat. . 73.03 72.31 75.99 72.57 62.58 10.00 71.6 20.96 18.88 17.11 20.05 22.32 3.00 18.8 5.41 7.41 5.77 6.81 8.68 S0.50 8.2 0.46 0.07 1.14 1.33 1.33 1.10 Pork (bacon), very fat* . Mackerel * 6.42 6.5 1.4 The vegetable foods are distinguished, as a rule, by their large percentage in carbohydrates and the relatively small amounts of proteins and fats, as seen, for example, in the composition of rice, corn, wheat, and potatoes. Nevertheless, it will be noticed that the proportion of protein in some of the vegetables is not at all insignifi- cant. They are characterized by their excess in carbohydrates rather than by a deficiency in proteins. The composition of peas and other leguminous foods is remarkable for the large percentage of protein, which exceeds that found in meats. Analyses such as are given here are indispensable in determining the true nutritive value of foods. Nevertheless, it must be borne in mind that the chemical composition of a food is not alone sufficient to determine its precise value in nutrition. It is obviously true that it is not what we eat, but what we digest and absorb, that is nutritious to the body; so that, in addition to determining the proportion of food- stuffs in any given food, it is necessary to determine to what extent the several constituents are digested. This factor can be obtained only by actual experiments. It may be said here, however, that in general the proteins of animal foods are more completely digested than arc those of. vegetables, owing chiefly to the fact that the Latter may contain a considerable amounl of indigestible cellulose, which tends to proted the protein from the action of the diges- tive secretions. In the animal foods, therefore, chemical analysis conic- nearer to expressing directly the nutritive value. Accessory Articles of Diet. -In addition to the foodstuffs proper, our foods contain numerous other substances which in one way or another are useful in nutrition, although not abso- lutely necessary. These substances, differing in nature and importance, may be classified under the three heads of: Flavor : the various oils or esters tliut give odor and taste to foods. Condimenl : pepper, salt, mustard, etc. Stimulants: alcohol, tea, coffee, cocoa, etc. * Atwater: "The Chemistry of Foods and Nutrition," 1887. COMPOSITION OF FOOD AND ACTION OF ENZYMES. 715 The specific influence of these substances in digestion and nutri- tion is considered in the section on Nutrition. The Chemical Changes of the Foodstuffs during Digestion. — The physiology of digestion consists chiefly in the study of the chemical changes that the food undergoes during its passage through the alimentary canal. It happens that these chemical changes are of a peculiar character. The peculiarity is due to the fact that the changes of digestion are effected through the agency of a group of bodies known as enzymes, or unorganized ferments, whose chemical action is more obscure than that of the ordinary reagents -with which we have to deal. It will save repetition to give here certain general facts that are known with reference to these bodies, reserving for later treatment the details of the action of the specific enzymes found in the different digestive secretions. ENZYMES AND THEK ACTION. Historical. — The term fermentation and the idea that it is meant to convey has varied greatly during the course of years. The word at first was applied to certain obvious and apparently spon- taneous changes in organic materials which are accompanied by the liberation of bubbles of gas: such, for instance, as the alcoholic fermentations, in which alcohol is formed from sugar; the acid fer- mentations, as in the souring of milk; and the putrefactive fer- mentations, by means of which animal substances are disintegrated, with the production of offensive odors. These mysterious phenom- ena excited naturally the interest of investigators, and with the development of chemical knowledge numerous other processes were discovered which resemble the typical fermentations in that they seem to be due to specific agents whose mode of action differs from the usual chemical reactions, especially in the fact that the causa- tive agent itself, or the ferment as it is called, is not destroyed or used up in the reaction. Thus it was discovered that germinating barley grains contain a something which can be extracted by water « and which can convert starch into sugar (Kirchhoff, 1814). Later this substance was separated by precipitation with alcohol and was given the name of diastase (Payen and Persoz, 1833). Schwann in 1836 demonstrated the existence of a ferment (pepsin) in gastric juice capable of acting upon albuminous substances, and a number of similar bodies were soon discovered: trypsin in the pancreatic juice, amygdalin, invertin, ptyalin, etc. These substances were all designated as ferments, and their action was compared to that of the alcoholic fermentation in yeast, the process of putrefaction, etc. Naturally very many theories have been proposed regarding the cause of the processes of fermentation. lor the historical develop- 716 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. ment and interrelation of these theories references must be made to special works.* It is sufficient here to say that the brilliant work of Pasteur established the fact that the fermentations in the old sense — alcoholic, acid, and putrefactive — are due to the presence and activity of living organisms. He showed, moreover, that many diseases are likewise due to the activity of minute living organisms, and thus justified the view held by some of the older physicians that there is a close similarity in the processes of fer- mentation and disease. The clear demonstration of the importance of living organisms in some fermentations and the equally clear proof of the existence of another group of ferment actions in which living material is not directly concerned led to a classification which is used even at the present day. This classification divided fer- ments into two great groups : the living or organized ferments, such as the yeast cell, bacteria, etc.; and the non-living or unorganized ferments, such as pepsin, trypsin, etc., which later were generally designated as enzymes (Kuhne). The separation appeared to be entirely satisfactory until Buchner (1897) showed that an unor- ganized ferment, an enzyme (zymase) capable of producing alcohol from sugar, may be extracted from yeast cells. Later the same observer (1903) succeeded in extracting enzymes from the lactic- acid-producing bacteria and the acetic-acid-producing bacteria which are capable of giving the same reactions as the living bacteria. These discoveries indicate clearly that there is no essential difference between the activity of living and non-living ferments. The so- called organized ferments probably produce their effects not by virtue of their specific life-metabolism, but by the manufacture within their substance of specific enzymes. If we can accept this conclusion, then the general explanation of fermentation is to be sought in the nature of the enzymatic processes. Within recent years the study of the enzymes has attracted especial attention. The general point of view regarding their mode of action that is fcnosl frequently met with to-day is that advocated especially by Ostwald. He assumes, reviving an older view (Berzelius), ili.it the ferment actions are similar to those of catalysis. By catalysis chemists designated a species of reaction which is brought aboui by the mere contact or presence of certain substances, the catalyzers. Thus, hydrogen and oxygen ;it ordinary temperatures do not combine to form water, but if spongy platinum is present the two gases unite readily. The platinum does not enter into the reaction, at least it undergoes no change, and it is said, therefore, * Consult, Clrecri. "The Soluble Ferments and Fermentations, " 1899; Effront, "Enzymes and their Applications" (translation by Prescott), 1902; Oppenheimer, "Die Fermente and ihre Wirkungen," second edition, 1903; Moore, in "Recent Advances in Physiology and Biochemistry, London and New York," 1906: and " rlandbuch der Biochemie," vol. L L908. COMPOSITION OF FOOD AND ACTION OF ENZYMES. 717 to act by catalysis. Many similar catalytic reactions are known, and the chemists have reached the important generalization that in such reactions the catalyzer, platinum in the above instance, simply hastens a process which would occur without it, but much more slowly. A catalyzer is a substance, therefore, that alters the velocity of a reaction, but does not initiate it. This idea is illustrated very clearly by the catalysis of hydrogen peroxid. This substance decomposes spontaneously into water and oxygen accord- ing to the reaction H202 = H20 + 0, but the decomposition is greatly hastened by the presence of a catalyzer. Thus, Bredig has shown that platinum in very fine suspension, so-called colloidal solution, exerts a marked accelerating influence upon this reaction; one part of the colloidal platinum to 350 million parts of water may still exercise a perceptible effect. The blood and aqueous ex- tracts of various tissues also catalyze the hydrogen peroxid readily, and this effect has been attributed to the action of an enzyme (cata- lase). The view has been proposed, therefore, that the enzymes of the body act like the catalyzers of inorganic origin: they influence the velocity of certain special reactions. Such a general conception as this unifies the whole subject of fermentation and holds out the hope that the more precise investigations that are possible in the case of the inorganic catalyzers will eventually lead to a better under- standing of the underlying physical causes of fermentation. It should be borne in mind, however, that some of the best known of the ferment actions of the body, such as the peptic or tryptic digestion of protein, fit into this view only theoretically and by analogy. As a matter of fact, albumins at ordinary temperatures do not split up spontaneously into the products formed by the action of pepsin; if we consider that the pepsin simply accelerates a reaction already taking place, it must be stated that this reaction at ordinary temperatures is infinitely slow, — that is, practically does not occur. At higher temperatures, however, similar decompositions of al- bumin may be obtained without the presence of an enzyme. Reversible Reactions. — It has been shown that under proper conditions many chemical reactions are reversible, — that is, may take place in opposite directions. For instance, acetic acid and ethyl-alcohol brought together react with the production of ethyl- acetate and water: CH3COOH + C2H5OH = CH3COOC2H5 + H20. Acetic acid. Alcohol. Ethyl-acetate. Water. On the other hand, when ethyl-acetate and water are brought together they react with the formation of some acetic acid and ethyl-alcohol, so that the reaction indicated in the above equation 718 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. takes place in opposite directions, figuratively speaking, — a fact which may be indicated by a symbol of this kind: CH3COOH + C2H5OH ^± CH3COOC2H5 + H20. It is evident that in a reversible reaction of this sort the opposite changes will eventually strike an equilibrium, the solution or mix- ture will contain some of all four substances, and this equilib- rium will remain constant as long as the conditions are unchanged. If the conditions are altered, however, — if, for example, some of the substances formed are removed or the mixture is altered as to its concentration, — then the reaction will proceed unequally in the two directions until a new equilibrium is established. The importance, in the present connection, of this conception of reversibility of reac- tions is found in the fact that a number of the catalytic reactions are also reversible. The catalyzer may not only accelerate a reac- tion between two substances, but may also accelerate the recom- position of the products into the original substances. An excellent instance of this double effect has been obtained by Kastle and Loevenhart in experiments upon one of the enzymes of the animal body, lipase. Lipase is the enzyme which in the body acts upon the neutral fats, converting them into fatty acids and glycerin, — a process that takes place as a usual if not necessary step in the diges- tion and absorption of fats. The authors above named* made use of a simple ester analogous to the fats, ethyl-butyrate, and showed that lipase causes not only an hydrolysis of this substance into ethyl- alcohol and butyric acid, but also a synthesis of the two last-named substances into ethyl-butyrate and water. The reaction effected by the lipase is therefore reversible and may be expressed as: C3H7COOC2H5 + H20 ^± C3H7COOH + C2H5OH. Ethyl-butyrate. Water. Butyric acid. Ethyl-alcohol. Lipase is capable of exerting probably a similar reversible reaction on the fats in the body. Assuming the existence of such an action in the body, it is possible to explain not only the digestion of fats, but also their formation in the tissues and their absorption from the tissues during starvation. That is, according to the conditions of concentration, etc., one and the same enzyme may cause a splitting up of the neutral fat into fatty acids and glycerin or a storing up of neutral fat by the synthesis of fatty acid and glycerin. In the subcutaneous I issues, therefore, fat may be stored, to a certain point, or, if the conditions are altered, the fat that is there may be changed over to the fatty acids and glycerin and be oxidized in the body as food. * Kastle ami Loevenhart, "American Chemical Journal," 24, 401, 1900. See also Loevenhart, " American Physiological Journal," 0, 331, 1902. COMPOSITION OF FOOD AND ACTION OF ENZYMES. 719 A similar reversibility has been shown for some of the other enzymes of the body (maltase by Hill, 1898), but whether or not all of them will be shown to possess this power under the conditions of temperature, etc., that prevail in the body can only be determined by actual experiments. The Specificity of Enzymes. — A most interesting feature of the activity of enzymes is that it is specific. The enzymes that act upon the carbohydrates are not capable of affecting the pro- teins or fats, and vice versa. So in the fermentation of closely related bodies such as the double sugars, the enzyme that acts upon the maltose is not capable of affecting the lactose; each re- quires seemingly its own specific enzyme. In fact, there is no clear proof that any single enzyme can produce more than one kind of ferment action. If in any extract or secretion two or more kinds of ferment action can be demonstrated, the tendency at present is to attribute these different activities to the existence of separate and specific enzymes. The pancreatic juice, for example, splits proteins, starches, and fats and curdles milk, and there are assumed to be four different enzymes present, — namely, trypsin, diastase, lipase, and rennin. So if an extract containing diastase is also capable of decomposing hydrogen peroxid it is believed that this latter effect is due to the existence of a special enzyme, catalase. It seems quite probable that this specificity of the different enzymes may be related, as Fischer * has suggested, to the geometrical struc- ture of the substance acted upon. Each ferment is adapted to act upon or become attached to a molecule with a certain definite structure, — fitted to it, in fact, as a key to its lock. In this respect the action of the so-called hydrolytic enzymes differs markedly from the dilute acids or alkalies which hydrolyze many different substances without indication of any specificity. Attention has been called to the fact that this adaptibility of enzymes to certain specific struc- tures in the molecules acted upon resembles closely the specific activity of the toxins, and many useful and suggestive com- parisons may be drawn between the mode of action of enzymes and toxins. It has become customary to speak of the substance upon which an enzyme acts as its substrate, and it has been assumed that the action of the enzyme, like that of the toxins, takes place in two stages; first, the combination of the enzyme and the substrate; second, the breaking down of this compound to give the final products of the reaction. There is some reason for believing that these two stages may be separated, and that enzymes which on account of certain conditions, such as heating, have lost their power of decomposing the substrate, may still have the power of combining with it. Toxins showing a similar * Fischer, "Zeitschrift f. physiolog. Chemie, " 26, 71, 1898. 720 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. property are designated as toxoids, and for the enzyme in this condition the term zymoid has been suggested (Bayliss). Definition and Classification of Enzymes (Ferments). — On the basis of the considerations presented in the preceding paragraphs Oppenheimer suggests the following definition: An enzyme is a substance, produced by living cells, which acts by catalysis. The enzyme itself remains unchanged in this process, and it acts specifi- cally.— that is, each enzyme exerts its activity only upon substances whose molecules have a certain definite structural and stereochemi- cal arrangement. The enzymes of the body are organic substances of a colloid structure whose chemical composition is unknown. With regard to the names and classification of the different enzymes, much difficulty is experienced. There is no consensus among workers as to the system to be followed. Duclaux has sug- gested that an enzyme be designated by the name of the body on which its action is exerted, and that all of them be given the termina- tion ase. The enzyme acting on fat on this system would be named lipase; that on starch, amylase; that on maltose, maltase, etc. The suggestion has been followed in part only, the older enzymes which were first discovered being referred to most frequently under their original names. An entirely satisfactory classification is impossible at present. Having in mind only the needs of animal physi- ology, the following classification will be used in the treatment of the subjects of digestion and nutrition: 1. The proteolytic or protein-splitting enzymes. Examples: pepsin of gastric juice, trypsin of pancreatic juice. They cause a hydro- lytic cleavage of the protein molecule. 2. The amylolytic or starch-splitting enzymes. Examples: ptyalin or salivary diastase, amylase, or pancreatic diastase. Their action is closely similar to dial of the classical enzyme of this group — dias- tase— found in germinating barley grains. They cause a hydrolytic cleavage of the starch molecule. 3. The lipolytic or fat-splitting enzymes. Example: the lipase found in the pancreatic secretion, in the liver, connective tissues, blood, etc. They cause a hydrolytic cleavage of the fat molecule. 4. The sugar-splitting enzymes. These again fall into two subgroups: (a) The inverting enzymes, which convert I he double sugars or di- saccharids into the monosaccharids. Examples: maltase, which splits maltose to dextrose; invertase, which splits cane-sugar to dextrose ami Levulose; and lad, ase, which splits milk-sugar (lactose) to dextrose ami galactose. (/') The enzymes which split the m on o- i hands. There is evidence of the presence in the tissues of an enzyme capable of splitting the sugar of the blood and tissues (dexl rose) into lactic acid. 5. The coagulating enzymes, which convert soluble to insoluble pro- tein . I . cample: The coagulation of the casein of milk by rennin. ft. The oxidizing enzymes or oxidases. A group of enzymes which set up oxidation processes. Some of the details of the activity of these enzymes an- considered in the discussion of physiological oxidations (p. 918). 7. The deamidizing enzymes, such as adenase and guanase, which by hydrolyl IC cleavage split off an N 1 1., group as ammonia. COMPOSITION OF FOOD AND ACTION OF ENZYMES. 721 The enzymes contained in the first, second, third, and fourth (a) of these groups are the ones that play the chief roles in the digestive processes, and it will be noticed that they all act by hydrolysis, — ■ that is, they cause the molecules of the substance to undergo de- composition or cleavage by a reaction with water. Thus, in the conversion of maltose to dextrose by the action of maltase the re- action may be expressed so: C12H22Ou + H20 = C6H]206 + C6H1206. Maltose. Dextrose. Dextrose. And the hydrolysis of the neutral fats by lipase may be expressed so: C3H5(C18H3502)3 + 3H20 = C,Hs(OH)3 + 3(C18H30O2). Tristearin. Glycerin. Stearic acid. General Properties of Enzymes. — The specific reactions of the various enzymes of the body are referred to under separate heads. The following general characteristics may be noted briefly: Solubility. — Most of the enzymes are soluble in water or salt solutions, or in glycerin. By these means they may be extracted conveniently from the various tissues. In some cases, however, such simple methods do not suffice ; the enzyme is destroj^ed in the process of extraction, and to prove its presence pieces of the tissue or the juice pressed from the tissue must be employed. Temperature. — The body enzymes are characterized by the fact that they are destroyed by high temperatures (60° C. to 80° C.) and that their effect is retarded in part or entirely by low temperatures. Most of them show an optimum activity at temperatures approxi- mating that of the body. Precipitation. — The enzymes are precipitated from their solutions in part at least by excess of alcohol. This precipitation is frequently used in obtaining purified specimens of enzymes. The enzymes, moreover, show an interesting tendency to be carried down mechani- cally by flocculent precipitates produced in their solutions. If protein present in the solution is precipitated, for instance, the enzymes may be carried down with it in part. Incompleteness of their Action. — In any given mixture of a sub- stance and its enzyme the action of the latter is usually not com- plete,— that is, all of the substance does not disappear. An explana- tion for this fact has been found in the reversibility of the action of the enzyme. If the reaction proceeds in both directions, then evidently under fixed conditions a final equilibrium will be reached in which no further apparent change takes place, although in reality the condition is not one of rest, but of balance between opposing processes proceeding at a definite rate. Within the body itself, on the contrary, the action of an enzyme may be complete, since the products are removed by absorption and the possibility of a re- versed reaction is removed. 46 722 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. Active and Inactive Form. — In many cases it can be shown that the enzyme exists within the cell producing it in an inactive form or even when secreted it may still be inactive. This antecedent or inactive stage is usually designated as zymogen or proferment. The zymogen may be stored in the cell in the form of granules which are converted into active enzyme at the moment of secretion, or it may be secreted in inactive form and require the co-operation of some other substance before it is capable of effecting its normal reaction. In such cases the second substance is said to activate the enzyme. In connection with the process of activation various terms have been emplo)red to designate the substance responsible for the activation. According to a recent classification* it has been suggested that inorganic substances causing activation shall be designated simply as activators, while organic substances playing a similar role shall be named kinases. An example of the latter is found in the case of the enterokinase which activates the trypsin of the pancreatic secretion. PARTIAL LIST OF THE ENZYMES CONCERNED IN THE PROC- ESSES OF DIGESTION AND NUTRITION. -3 s Cm Enzyme. f Ptyalin (sali- vary diastase. Amylase (pancreatic diastase). Liver diastase. Muscle diastase. Invertase. Maltase. Lactase. Glycolytic? Lipase (steap- sin). Pepsin. Trypsin. Brepsin. Group of auto- lytic enzymes. Where Chiefly Found. Salivary secretion. Pancreatic secre- tion. Liver. Muscles. Small intestine. Small intestine, salivary and pancreatic se- cretion. Small intestine. Muscles? Pancreatic secre- 1 ion, fat tissues, blood, etc. Gastric juice. Pancreatic juice. Small intestine. Tissues generally. imuely in '' Elandbucb der Biochemie Action. Converts starch to sugar (maltose). Converts starch to sugar (maltose). Converts glycogen to dex- trose. Converts glycogen to dex- trose. Converts cane-sugar to dextrose and levulose. Converts maltose to dex- trose. Converts lactose to dex- trose and galactose. Splits and oxidizes dex- trose. Splits neutral fats to fatty acids and glycerin. Converts proteins to pep- tones and pro I coses. Splits proteins into sim- pler crystalline prod- ucts. Splits peptones into sim- pler products. Splits proteins into nitrog- enous bases and amino* bodies. 1908. COMPOSITION OF FOOD AND ACTION OF ENZYMES. 723 • -• m o3 C I p I Nuclease. Guanase. Adenase. Oxidases. Catalase. Pancreas, spleen, thymus, etc. Thymus, adrenals, pancreas. Spleen, pancreas, liver. Lungs, liver, mus- cle, etc. Many tissues. Splits nucleic acid with for- mation of purin bases, etc. Converts guanin to xan- thin by splitting off an NH2 group as ammonia (NH3). Converts adenin to hypo- xanthin by splitting off an NH, group as am- monia (NH3). Cause oxidation of organ- ic substances, as in the conversion of hypoxan- thin to xanthin and of xanthin to uric acid. Decomposes hydrogen peroxid. Chemical Composition of the Enzymes. — It was formerly believed that the enzymes belong to the group of proteins. They are formed from living matter, and their solutions as usually prepared give protein reactions. Increased study, however, has made this belief uncertain. The enzymes cling to the proteins when precipitated, and it seems possible that the protein reac- tions of their solutions may be due, therefore, to an incomplete purification. In fact, it is stated that solutions of some of the enzymes may be prepared which show ferment activity, but give no protein reactions. In this group may be included the lipase, diastase, invertase, pepsin, oxidase, and catalase. Appar- ently, however, all enzymes contain nitrogen and most of them also sulphur. They probably also contain some ash, especially calcium. Much of the older work upon the composition of supposedly purified preparations of enzymes is not accepted to-day, on the ground that the evidence for the purity of the preparations is insufficient. In spite, however, of the very great amount of attention that has been paid to these substances in recent years, there is at present no agreement as to their chemical structure. The statement made above that they are organic substances, derived from proteins and of a colloidal nature, is perhaps as much as can be said positively in regard to their chemical structure. CHAPTER XLI. THE SALIVARY GLANDS AND THEIR DIGESTIVE ACTION. The first of the secretions with which the food comes into contact is the saliva. This is a mixed secretion from the large salivary glands and the small unnamed mucous and serous glands that open into the mouth cavity. The Salivary Glands. — The salivary glands in man are three in number on each side — the parotid, the submaxillary, and the sublingual. The parotid gland communicates with the mouth by a large duct (Stenson's duct) which opens upon the inner surface of the cheek opposite the second molar tooth of the upper jaw. The submaxillary gland lies below the lower jaw, and its duct (Wharton's duct) opens into the mouth cavity at the side of the frenum of the tongue. The sublingual gland lies in the floor of the mouth to the side of the frenum and opens into the mouth cavity by a number (eight to twenty) of small ducts, known as the ducts of Rivinus. One larger duct that runs parallel with the duct of Whar- ton and opens separately into the mouth cavity is sometimes present in man. It is known as the duct of Bartholin and occurs normally in the dog. The course of the nerve fibers supplying the large salivary glands is interesting in view of the physiological results of their stimulation. The description here given applies especially to their arrangement in the dog. These glands receive their nerve supply from two general sources, — namely, the bulbar autonomics (or cerebral fibers) and the sympathetic autonomics. The parotid gland receives its bulbar autonomic fibers from the glossopharyngeal or ninth cranial nerve; they pass into a branch of this nerve known as the tympanic branch or nerve of Jacobson, thence to the small superficial pe- trosal nerve, through which they reach the otic ganglion. From this ganglion they pass (postganglionic fibers) by way of the auricu- lotemporal branch of the inferior maxillary division of the fifth cranial nerve to the parotid gland (Fig. 278). The sympathetic autonomics passto the superior cervical ganglion by way of the cervical sympathetic (Fig. Ill) and thence as postganglionic fibers in branches which accompany the arteries distributed to the gland. The bulbar autonomic supply for the submaxillary and sublingual 724 THE SALIVARY GLANDS. 725 glands arises from the brain in the facial nerve and passes out in the chorda tympani branch (Fig. 279) . This latter nerve, after emerging from the tympanic cavity through the Glaserian fissure, joins the Inferior?ftt!} Tetrous Sanation. Fig. 278.— Schematic representation of the course of the cerebral fibers to the parotid gland. lingual nerve. After running with this nerve for a short distance,, the secretory (and vasodilator) nerve fibers destined for the sub- maxillary and sublingual glands branch off and pass to the glands, Tnferioflttaxillarv JJBranehefj&! Sub- 7Haxilla.rtf i Su6 lin-yua-t- Gaiwlu ■^Branches to *Tongu£ Fig. 279. — Schematic representation of the course of the chorda tympani nerve to the submaxillary gland. following the course of the ducts. ' Where the chorda tympani fibers leave the lingual there is a small ganglion which has received the name of submaxillary ganglion. The nerve fibers to the glands 726 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. pass close to this ganglion, but Langley has shown that only those destined for the sublingual gland really connect with the nerve cells of the ganglion, and he suggests, therefore, that it should be called the sublingual instead of the submaxillary ganglion. The nerve fibers for the submaxillary gland make connections with nerve cells lying mainly within the hilus of the gland itself. The supply of sympathetic autonomics has the same general course as those for the parotid, — namely, through the cervical sympathetic to the superior cervical ganglion and thence to the glands. Histological Structure. — The salivary glands belong to the type of compound tubular glands. That is, the secreting portions are tubular in shape, although in cross-sections these tubes may pre- sent various outlines according as the plane of the section passes through them. The parotid is described usually as a typical serous or albuminous gland. Its secreting epithelium is composed of cells which in the fresh condition as well as in preserved specimens contain numerous fine granules and its secretion contains some albumin. The submaxillary gland differs in histology in different animals. In some, as the dog or cat, the secretory tubes are composed chiefly or exclusively of epithelial cells of the mucous type. In man the gland is of a mixed type, the secretory tubes containing both mucous and albuminous cells. The sublingual gland in man also contains both varieties of cells, although the mucous cells predominate. In accordance with these histological characteristics it is found that the secretion from the submaxillary and sublingual glands is thick and mucilaginous as compared with that from the parotid. In the mucous glands another variety of cell, the so-called demilunes or crescent cells, is frequently met with, and the physiological significance of these cells has been the subject of much discussion. The demilunes are crescent-shaped, granular cells lying between the mucous cells and the base- ment membrane, and not, in contact, therefore, with the central lumen of the tube. According to Heidenhain, these demilunes are for the purpose of replacing tin- mucous cells. In consequence of long-continued activity the mucous cells may disintegrate and disappear, and the demilunes then lop into new mucous cells. Another view is that the demilunes represent distind secretory cells of the albuminous type, while others assert that they are a specific type of cell with probably specific functions.* The salivary glands possess definite secretory nerves which when stimulated cause the formation of a secretion. This fact indicates that there must be a direct contact of some kind between the gland cells and the terminations of the secretory fibers. The ending of the nerve fibers in the submaxillary and sublingual glands has been de- scribed by a number of observers.f The accounts differ somewhat as to details of the finer anatomy, but if seems to be clearly established I hat f ho secretory fibers from the chorda tympani end first around the ♦ See Noll, "Archiv f. Physiologic," VMY>, suppl. volume, 166. fSee Huber, "Journal of Experimental Medicine," 1, 281, 1896. THE SALIVARY GLANDS. 727 intrinsic nerve ganglion cells of the glands (preganglionic fibers), and from these latter cells axons (postganglionic fibers) are distributed to the secreting cells, passing to these cells along the ducts. The nerve fibers terminate in a plexus upon the membrana propria of the alveoli, and from this plexus fine fibrils pass inward to end on and between the secreting cells. It would seem from these observations that the nerve fibrils do not penetrate or fuse with the gland cells, as was formerly supposed, but form a terminal network in contact with the cells, following thus the general schema for the connection between nerve fibers and peripheral tissues. Composition of the Secretion. — The saliva as it is found in the mouth is a colorless or opalescent, turbid, and mucilaginous liquid of weakly alkaline reaction and a specific gravity of about 1.003. It may contain numerous flat cells derived from the epithelium of the mouth, and the peculiar spherical cells known as salivary corpuscles, which seem to be altered leucocytes. The important constituents of the secretion are mucin, a diastatic enzyme known as ptyalin, maltase, traces of protein and of potassium sulphocyanid, and inorganic salts such as potassium and sodium chlorid, potassium sulphate, sodium carbonate, and calcium carbonate and phosphate. The carbonates are particularly abundant in the saliva, and the secretion in addition contains much carbon dioxid in solution. Thus, PfKiger found that 65 volumes per cent, of C02 might be obtained from the saliva, of which 42.5 per cent, was in the form of carbonates. The amount of C02 in solution and combined is an indication of the active chemical changes in the gland. Of the organic constituents of the saliva the protein exists in small and variable quantities, and its exact nature is not determined. The mucin gives to the saliva its ropy, mucilaginous character. This substance belongs to the group of combined proteins, glyco- proteins (see Appendix), consisting of a protein combined with a carbohydrate group. The most interesting constituent of the mixed saliva is the ptyalin or salivary diastase. This body belongs to the group of enzymes or unorganized ferments, whose general properties have been described. In some animals (dog) ptyalin seems to be normally absent from the fresh saliva. The secretions of the parotid and the submaxillary glands can be obtained separately by inserting a cannula into the openings of the ducts in the mouth, or, according to the method of Pawlow, by trans- ferring the end of the duct so that it opens upon the skin instead of in the mouth, making thus a salivary fistula. The secretion of the sublingual can only be obtained in sufficient quantities for analysis from the lower animals. Examination of the separate secretions shows that the main difference lies in the fact that the parotid saliva contains no mucin, while that of the submaxillary and especially of 728 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. the sublingual gland is rich in mucin. The parotid saliva of man seems to be particularly rich in ptyalin as compared with that of the submaxillary, while the secretion of the latter and that of the sub- lingual gland give a stronger alkaline reaction than the parotid saliva. The Secretory Nerves. — The existence of secretory nerves to the salivary glands was discovered by Ludwig in 1851. The discovery is particularly interesting in that it marks the beginning of our knowl- edge of this kind of nerve fiber. Ludwig found that stimulation of the chorda tympani nerve causes a flow of saliva from the submaxil- lar}- gland. He established also several important facts with regard to the pressure and composition of the secretion which will be referred to presently. It was afterward shown that the salivary glands receive a double nerve supply, — in part by way of the cervical sympathetic and in part through cerebral nerves. It was discovered also that not only are secretory fibers carried to the glands by these paths, but that vasomotor fibers are contained in the same nerves, and the arrangement of these latter fibers is such that the cerebral nerves contain vasodilator fibers that cause a dilatation of the small arteries in the glands and an accelerated blood-flow, while the sym- pathetic carries vasoconstrictor fibers whose stimulation causes a constriction of the small arteries and a diminished blood-flow. The effect of stimulating these two sets of fibers is found to vary somewhat in different animals. For purposes of description we may confine ourselves to the effects observed on dogs, since much of our funda- mental knowledge upon the subject is derived from Heidenhain's* experiments upon this animal. If the chorda tympani nerve is stimulated by weak induction shocks, the gland begins to secrete promptly, and the secretion, by proper regulation of the stimulation, may be kept up for hours. The secretion thus obtained is thin and watery, flows freely, is abundant in amount, and contains not more than 1 or 2 per cent, of total solids. At the same time there is an increased flow of blood through the gland. The whole gland takes on a redder hue, the veins are distended, and if cut the blood that flows from them is of a redder color than in the resting gland, and may show a distinct pulse — all of which points to a dilatation of the small arteries. If now the sympathetic fibers are stimulated, quite different results are obtained. The secretion is relatively small in amount, flows slowly, is thick and turbid, and may contain as much as 6 per cent, of total solids. At the same time the gland becomes pale, and if the veins be cut the flow from them is slower than in the resting gland, thus indicating that a vasoconstriction has occurred. The increased vascular supply to the gland accompanying the ♦"Pfluger'e Archiv fur die gesaiiinil.e Physiologic," 17, 1, 1878; also in Hermann' "Handbuch der Physiologic," is*;',, vol. v, part i. THE SALIVARY GLANDS. 729 abundant flow of "chorda saliva" and the diminished flow of blood during the scanty secretion of "sympathetic saliva" suggest naturally the idea that the whole process of secretion may be, at bottom, a vasomotor phenomenon, the amount of secretion depending only on the quantity and pressure of the blood flowing through the gland. It has been shown conclusively that this idea is erroneous and that definite secretory fibers exist. The following facts may be quoted in support of this statement: (1) Ludwig showed that if a mercury manometer is connected with the duct of the submaxillary gland and the chorda is then stimulated for a certain time, the pressure in the duct may become greater than the blood-pressure in the gland. This fact shows that the secretion is not derived entirely by processes of filtration from the blood. (2) If the blood-flow be shut off completely from the gland, stimulation of the chorda still gives a secretion for a short time. (3) If atropin is injected into the gland, stimulation of the chorda causes vascular dilatation, but no secretion. This may be explained by supposing that the atropin paralyzes the secretory, but not the dilator fibers. (4) Hydro- chlorate of quinin injected into the gland causes vascular dilatation, but no secretion. In this case the secretory fibers are still irritable, since stimulation of the chorda gives the usual secretion. A still more marked difference between the effect of stimulation of the cerebral and the sympathetic fibers may be observed in the case of the parotid gland in the dog. Stimulation of the cerebral fibers, in any part of their course, gives an abundant, thin, and watery saliva, poor in solid constituents. Stimulation of the sym- pathetic fibers alone (provided the cerebral fibers have not been stimulated shortly before and the tympanic nerve has been cut to prevent a reflex effect) gives usually no perceptible secretion at all. But in this last stimulation a marked effect is produced upon the gland, in spite of the absence of a visible secretion. This is shown by the fact that subsequent or simultaneous stimulation of the cerebral fibers causes a secretion very unlike that given by the cerebral fibers alone, in that it is very rich indeed in organic constituents. The amount of organic matter in the secretion may be tenfold that of the saliva obtained by stimulation of the cerebral fibers alone. Relation of the Composition of the Secretion to the Strength of Stimu- lation.— If the stimulus to the chorda is gradually increased in strength, care being taken not to fatigue the gland, the chemical composition of the secretion is found to change with regard to the relative amounts of the water, the salts, and the organic material. The water and the salts increase in amount with the increased strength of stimulus up to a certain maximal limit, which for the salts is about 0.77 per cent. It is important to observe that this effect may be obtained from a perfectly fresh gland as well as from a gland which had previously been secreting actively. With regard 730 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. to the organic constituents the precise result obtained depends on the condition of the gland. If previous to the stimulation the gland was in a resting condition and unfatigued, then increased strength of stimulation is followed at first by a rise in the percentage of organic constituents, and this rise in the beginning is more marked than in the case of the salts. But with continued stimulation the increase in organic material soon ceases, and finally the amount begins actually to diminish, and may fall to a low point in spite of the stronger stimulation. On the other hand, if the gland at the beginning of the experiment had been previously worked to a considerable extent, then an increase in the stimulating current, while it augments the amount of water and salts, either may have no effect at all upon the organic constituents or may cause only a temporary increase, quickly followed by a fall. Similar results may be obtained from stimulation of the cerebral nerves of the parotid gland. The above facts led Heidenhain to believe that the conditions determining the secretion of the organic material are different from those controlling the water and salts, and he gave a rational explanation of the differences observed, in his theory of trophic and secretory fibers. Theory of Trophic and Secretory Nerve Fibers. — This theory supposes that two physiological varieties of . nerve fibers are distrib- uted to the salivary glands. One of these varieties controls the secretion of the water and inorganic salts and its fibers may be called secretory fibers proper, while the other, to which the name trophic is given, causes the formation of the organic constituents of the secre- tion, probably by a direct influence on the metabolism of the cells. Were the trophic fibers to act alone, the organic products would be formed within the cell, but there would be no visible secretion, and this is the hypothesis which Heidenhain uses to explain the results of the experiment described above upon stimulation of the sympathetic fibers to the parotid of the dog. In this animal, apparently, the sympathetic branches to the parotid contain exclusively or almost exclusively trophic fibers, while in the cerebral branches both trophic and secretory fibers proper are present. The results of stimulation of the cerebral and sympathetic branches to the submaxillary gland of the same animal may be explained in terms of this theory by supposing that in the latter nerve trophic fibers preponderate, and in the former the secretory fibers proper. It is obvious that this anatomical separation of the two sets of fibers along the cerebral and sympathetic paths may be open to individual variations, and that dogs may be found in which the sym- pathetic branches to the parotid glands contain secretory fibers proper, and therefore give some flow of secretion on stimulation. These variations might also be expected to be more marked when animals of different groups are compared. Thus, Langley* finds ♦ "Journal of Physiology," I, 96, L878. THE SALIVARY GLANDS. 731 that in cats the sympathetic saliva from the submaxillary gland is less viscid than the chorda saliva, — just the reverse of what occurs in the dog. To apply Heidenhain's theory to this case it is necessary to assume that in the cat the trophic fibers run chiefly in the chorda. The way in which the trophic fibers act has been briefly indicated. They may be supposed to set up metabolic changes in the proto- plasm of the cells, leading to the formation of certain definite prod- ucts, such as mucin or ptyalin. That such changes do occur is abundantly shown by microscopical examination of the resting and the active gland, the details of which will be given presently. In general, these changes may be supposed to be catabolic in nature; that is, they consist in a disassociation or breaking down of the complex living material, with the formation of the simpler and more stable organic constituents of the secretion. That these changes involve processes of oxidation is shown by the fact that during activity the gland takes up more oxygen and gives off more carbon dioxid. There is evidence to show that these gland cells during activity form fresh material from the nourishment supplied by the blood; that is, that anabolic or building-up processes occur along with the catabolic changes. The latter are the more obvious, and are the changes which are usually associated with the action of the trophic nerve fibers. It is possible, also, that the anabolic or growth changes may be under the control of separate fibers, for which the name anabolic fibers would be appropriate. Satis- factory proof of the existence of a separate set of anabolic fibers has not yet been furnished. The method of action of the secretory fibers proper is difficult to understand. At present the theories suggested are entirely specula- tive. Experiments have shown that the amount of water given off from the blood during secretion is somewhat greater than the amount contained in the saliva,* and there is reason to believe that the difference between the two is accounted for by an increase in the flow of lymph from the gland during activity. A satisfactory explanation of the causes leading to and controlling the flow of water cannot yet be given. In a general way it has been assumed that the effect of the nerve impulses is to cause the production of substances within the cells whereby their osmotic pressure is increased, and a stream of water is set up from the blood in the capillaries toward the gland cells, but it cannot be said that this assumption has been supported by the experiments so far made.f We must limit ourselves to the more general statement that the activity of the cells themselves initiates and controls the flow of water. *Barcroft, "Journal of Physiology," 1900, 25, 479. f Carlson, Greer, and Becht, "American Journal of Physiology,'' 19, 360, 1907. 76Z PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION . AND SECRETION. Histological Changes During Activity. — The cells of both the albuminous and mucous glands undergo distinct histological changes in consequence of prolonged activity, and these changes may be recognized both in preparations from the fresh gland and in preserved specimens. In the parotid gland Heidenhain studied the changes in stained sections after hardening in alcohol. In the resting gland the cells are compactly filled with granules that stain readily and are imbedded in a clear ground substance that does not stain. The nucleus is small and more or less irregular in outline. After stimulation of the tympanic nerve the cells show but little alteration, but stimula- tion of the sympathetic produces a marked change. The cells become smaller, the nuclei more rounded, and the granules more closely packed. This last appearance seems, however, to be .;:-V. m> D Fir. 280. — Parotid gland of the rabbit in a fresh state, showing portions of the secret- ing tubules: I, In ■> re ting Condition; li, after secretion caused l>y pilocarpin; C, after ecretion, pilocarpin and stimulation of sympathetic; D, after lonjr-eontinued stimulation of sympathetic. — (After Langley.) due to the hardening reagents used. A truer picture of what occurs may be obtained from a study of sections of the fresh gland. Lang- ley,* who first used this method, describes his results as follows: When fche animal is in ;i fasting condition the cells have a granular appearance throughout their substance, the outlines of the different cells being faintly marked by light lines (Fig. 280, A). When the gland i- made t<> secrete by giving the animal food, by injecting pilocarpin, or by stimulating the sympathetic nerves, the granules begin to disappear from the outer borders of the cells (Fig. 280, B), ♦ "Journal of Physiology, " 2, 2G0, 187!). THE SALIVARY GLANDS. 733 so that each cell now shows an outer, clear border and an inner granular one. If the stimulation is continued the granules become fewer in number and are collected near the lumen and the margins of the cells, the clear zone increases in extent, and the cells become smaller (Fig. 280, C, D). Evidently the granular material is used in some way to make the organic material of the secretion. Since the ptyalin is a conspicuous organic constituent of the secretion, it is assumed that the granules in the resting gland contain the ptyalin, or rather the preliminary material from which the ptyalin is con- structed during the act of secretion. On this latter assumption the granules are frequently spoken of as zymogen granules. During the act of secretion two distinct processes seem to be going on in the cell, leaving out of consideration, for the moment, the secretion of the water and the salts. In the first place, the zymogen granules undergo a change such that they are forced or dissolved out of the cell, and, second, a constructive metabolism or anabolism is set up, leading to the formation of new pro- toplasmic material from the substances contained in the blood and lymph. The new material thus formed is the clear, non- granular substance, which appears first toward the basal sides of the cells. We may sup- pose that the clear sub- stance during the resting periods undergoes meta- bolic changes, whether of a, catabolic or anabolic character can not be safely asserted, leading to the formation of new granules, and the cells are again ready to form a secretion of normal composition. It should be borne in mind that in these experiments the glands were stimulated beyond normal limits. Under ordinary conditions the cells are probably never depleted of their granular material to the extent represented in the figures. In the cells of the mucous glands changes equally marked may be observed after prolonged activity. In stained sections of the resting gland the cells are large and clear (Fig. 281), with flattened nuclei placed well toward the base of the cell. When the gland is made to secrete the nuclei become more spherical and lie more toward the middle of the cell, and the cells themselves become distinctly smaller. After prolonged secretion the changes become more marked (Fig. 282) and, according to Heidenhain, some of the Fig. 281. — Mucous gland: submaxillary of dog ing stage. 734 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. Fig. 282. — Mucous gland: submaxillary of clog after eight hours' stimulation of the chorda tympani. mucous cells may break down completely. According to most of the later observers, however, the mucous cells do not actually dis- integrate, but form again new material during the period of rest, as in the case of the goblet cells of the intestine. In the mucous as in the albuminous cells observations upon pieces of the fresh gland seem to give more reliable results than those upon preserved specimens. Langley* has shown that in the fresh mucous cells of the submax- illary gland numerous large granules may be discovered, about 125 to 250 to a cell. These granules are comparable to those found in the goblet cells, and may be inter- preted as consisting of mu- cin or some preparatory material from which mucin is formed. The granules are sensitive to reagents; addition of water causes them to swell tip and dis- appear. It may be as- sumed that this happens during secretion, the gran- ules becoming converted to a mucin mass which is extruded from the cell. Action of Atropin, Pilocarpin, and Nicotin upon the Secre- tory Nerves. — The action of drugs upon the salivary glands and their secretions belongs properly to pharmacology, but the effects of the three drugs mentioned are so decided that they have a peculiar physiological interest. Atropin in small doses injected either into the blood or into the gland duct prevents the action of the cerebral autonomic fibers (tympanic nerve or chorda tympani) upon the glands. This effect may be explained by assuming that the atropin paralyzes the endings of the cerebral fibers in the glands. That it does not act directly upon the gland cells themselves seems to be assured by the interesting fact that, with doses sufficient to throw out entirely the secreting action of the cerebral fibers, the sympathetic fibers are still effective when stimulated, l'ilocarpin has directly the opposite effect to atropin. In minimal doses it sets up a continuous secretion of saliva, which may be explained upon the supposition that it stimulates the endings of the secretory fibers in the gland. Within certain limits these drugs antagonize each other, that Is, the effect of pilocarpin may be removed by the sub- Bequent application of atropin, and vice versa. Nicotin, according to the experiments of Langley,t prevents the ad ton of the secretory • "Journal of Physiology," L0, 133, L889. t "Proceedings of the Royal Society," London, 46,423, J 889. THE SALIVARY GLANDS. 735 nerves, not by affecting the gland cells or the endings of the nerve fibers around them, but by paralyzing the connections between the nerve fibers and the ganglion cells through which the fibers pass On their way to the gland, — that is, the connection between the pre- ganglionic and postganglionic fibers. If, for example, the superior cervical ganglion is painted with a solution of nicotin, stimulation of the cervical sympathetic below the gland gives no secretion; stim- ulation, however, of the fibers in the ganglion or between the ganglion and gland gives the usual effect. By the use of this drug Langley is led to believe that the cells of the so-called submaxillary ganglion are really intercalated in the course of the fibers to the sublingual gland, while the nerve cells with which the submaxillary fibers make connection are found chiefly in the hilus of the gland itself. Paralytic Secretion. — A remarkable phenomenon in connection with the salivary glands is the so-called paralytic secretion. It has been known for a long time that if the chorda tympani is cut the submaxillary gland after a certain time, one to three days, begins to secrete slowly, and the secretion continues uninterruptedly for a long period — as long, perhaps, as several weeks — and eventually the gland itself undergoes atrophy. Langley states that section of the chorda on one side is followed by a continuous secretion from the glands on both sides; the secretion from the gland of the opposite side he designates as the antiparalytic or antilytic secretion. After section of the chorda the nerve fibers peripheral to the section degenerate, the process being completed within a few days. These fibers, how- ever, do not run directly to the gland cell; they terminate in end arborizations around sympathetic nerve cells placed somewhere along their course, — in the sublingual ganglion, for instance, or within the gland substance itself. It is the axons from these second nerve units that end around the secreting cells. Langley has accumulated some facts to show that within the period of continuance of the paralytic secretion (five to six weeks) the fibers of the sympathetic cells are still irritable to stimulation. He is inclined to believe, therefore, that the continuous secretion is due to a continuous excitation, from some cause, of the local nervous mechanism in the gland. A natural extension of this yiew which has been suggested (Parlow) is that, normally the activity of the sympathetic cells or of the secreting cells is kept in check by inhibitory fibers. After section of the chorda the action of these fibers falls out and the secre- tion continues until the glandular tissue undergoes atrophy. On the histological side it is stated* that after section of the chorda the resulting degenerative changes affect only the cytoplasm, while after the section of the sympathetic the nuclei of the cells are affected, and, indeed, to some extent on the sound as well as on the injured side. * Gerhardt, "Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 97, 317, 1903. 736 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. Normal Mechanism of Salivary Secretion. — Under normal con- ditions the flow of saliva from the salivary glands is the result of a reflex stimulation of the secretory nerves. The sensory fibers concerned in this reflex must be chiefly fibers of the glossopharyn- geal and lingual nerves supplying the mouth and tongue. Sapid bodies and various other chemical or mechanical stimuli applied to the tongue or mucous membrane of the mouth produce a flow of saliva. The normal flow during mastication must be effected by a reflex of this kind, the sensory im- pulse being carried to a center and thence transmitted through the efferent nerves to the glands. It is found that section of the chorda prevents the reflex, in spite of the fact that the sympathetic fibers are still intact. No satisfactory explanation of the normal functions of the secretory fibers in the sympathetic has yet been given. Various authors have suggested that possibly the three large salivary glands respond normally to different stimuli. This view has been supported by Pawlow, who reports that in the dog at least the parotid and the submaxillary may react quite differently. When fistulas were made of the ducts of these glands it was found that the submaxillary responded readily to a great num- ber of stimuli, such as the sight of food, chewing of meats, acids, etc. The parotid, on the contrary, seemed to react only when dry food, dry powdered meat, or bread was placed in the mouth. Dryness in this case appeared to be the efficient stimulus. Pawlow lays great stress upon the adaptability of the secretion of saliva to the character of the material chewed. Dry, solid food stimulates a large flow of saliva, such as is necessary in order to chew it properly and to form it into a bolus for swallowing. Foods containing much water, on the contrary, excite but little flow of saliva. If one places a handful of clean stones in the mouth of a dog he will move them around with his tongue for a while and then drop them from his mouth; but little or no saliva is secreted. If the same material is given in the form of fine sand a rich flow of saliva is produced, and the necessity for the reflex is evident in this case, since otherwise the material could not be conveniently removed from the mouth. Such adaptations must be regarded from the physiological point of view as special reflexes depending upon some difference in the nervous mechanism set into play.* Since the flow of saliva is normally a definite reflex, we should expect a distinct salivary secretion center. This center has been located by physiological means in the medulla oblongata; its exact position is no! clearly defined, but possibly it is represented by the nuclei of origin of the secretory fibers which leave the medulla by way of the facial and glossopharyngeal nerves. Owing to the wide; connections of nerve cells in the central nervous system, we should expect, this center to be affected by stimuli from various sources. •See Pawlow, "The Work of the Digestive Glands," translation by Thompson, London, 1902; also " Ergebnisse der Physiologie," vol hi., part i, J '.in I . and " Archive internationaies de physiologie," 1, 119, 1904. THE SALIVARY GLANDS. 737 As a matter of fact, it is known that the center and through it the glands may be called into activity by stimulation of the sensory fibers of the sciatic, splanchnic, and particularly the vagus nerves. So, too, various psychical acts, such as the thought of savory food and the feeling of nausea preceding vomiting, may be accompanied by a flow of saliva, the effect in this case being due probably to stimula- tion of the secretion center by nervous impulses descending from the higher nerve centers. Lastly, the medullary center may be inhibited as well as stimulated. The well-known effect of fear, embarrassment, or anxiety in producing a parched throat may be explained in this way as due to the inhibitory action of nerve impulses arising in the cerebral centers. Electrical Changes in the Gland during Activity. — It has been shown that the salivary as well as other glands suffer certain changes in electrical potential during activity which are comparable in a gen- eral way to the "action currents" observed in muscles and nerves.* The Digestive Action of Saliva — Ptyalin. — The digestive action proper of the saliva is limited to the starchy food. In human beings and most mammals the saliva contains an active enzyme belonging to the group of diastases and designated usually as ptyalin or salivary diastase. It may be prepared in purified form from saliva by precipitation with alcohol, but its chemical nature, like that of the other enzymes, is still an unsolved problem. Saliva or preparations of ptyalin act readily upon boiled starch, converting it into sugar and dextrin. This action may be demonstrated very readily by holding a little starch paste or starchy food, such as boiled potatoes, in the mouth for a few moments. If the solution is then examined the presence of sugar is readily shown by its reducing action on solutions of copper sulphate (Fehling's solution). There is no doubt that the action of ptyalin upon the starch is hydrolytic. Under the influence of the enzyme the starch molecules take up water and undergo cleavage into simpler molecules. The steps in the process and the final products have been investigated by a very large number of workers, but much yet remains in doubt. The following points seem to be determined: The end-result of the reaction is the formation of maltose, a disaccharid, having the general formula C12H22Ou, and some form of dextrin, a non-crystallizable poly- saccharid. When the digestion is effected in a vessel some dextrose (C6H1206) may be found among the products, but this is explained on the assumption that there is present in the saliva some maltase, an enzyme capable of splitting maltose into dextrose. So far as the ptyalin itself is concerned, its specific action is to convert starch to maltose and dextrin. It seems very certain, however, that a number *See Biedennann, " Electro-physiology," translation by Welby, London, 1896. 47 738 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION" AND SECRETION. of intermediate products are formed consisting of a variety of dex- trins, so that the hydrolysis probably takes place in successive stages. There is little agreement as to the exact nature of the in- termediate dextrins. The following facts, however, may be easily demonstrated in a salivary digestion carried on in a vessel and ex- amined from time to time. The starch at first gives its deep-blue reaction with iodin; later, instead of a blue, a red reaction is obtained with iodin, and this has been attributed to a special form of dextrin, erythrodextrin, so named on account of its red reaction. Still later tins reaction fails and chemical examination shows the presence of maltose and a form of dextrin which gives no color reaction with iodin and is therefore named achroodextrin. While the number of intermediate products may be large, the main result of the action of the ptyalin is expressed by the following simple schema: Starch<^Ial*?se- , . . .Maltose. The products formed in this reaction are probably not absorbed as such. The absorption takes place mainly no doubt after the food reaches the small intestine, and we have evidence, as will be stated, that before absorption the maltose and the dextrin are acted upon by the inverting enzymes (maltase) and converted into the simple sugar, dextrose. The ptyalin digestion seems therefore to be pre- paratory, and the combined action of ptyalin and maltase is necessary to get the starch into a condition ready for nutrition. By way of comparison it is interesting to remember that when starch is boiled with dilute acids it is hydrolyzed at once to dextrose. A question of practical importance is as to how far salivary digestion affects the starchy foods under usual circumstances. The chewing process in the mouth thoroughly mixes the food and saliva, or should do so, but the bolus is swallowed much too quickly to enable the enzyme to complete its action. In the stomach the gastric juice is sufficiently acid to destroy the ptyalin, and it was therefore supposed formerly that salivary digestion is promptly arrested on the entrance of the food into the stomach, and is therefore normally of but little value as a digestive process. < )ur recent increase in knowledge regarding the conditions in the stomach (p. 698) shows, on the contrary, that Borne of the food in an ordinary meal may remain in the fundic end of fh^ stomach for an hour or more untouched by the acid secretion. There is every reason to believe, therefore, that salivary digestion may he carried on in the stomach to an important extent. Conditions Influencing the Action of Ptyalin.- Temperature. — As in the case of the other enzymes, ptyalin is very susceptible to eh; lilacs of temperature. At 0° C. its activity is said to he suspended entirely. The intensity of its action increases with increase of THE SALIVARY GLANDS. 739 temperature from this point, and reaches its maximum at about 40° C. If the temperature is raised much beyond this point, the action decreases, and at from 65° to 70° C. the enzyme is destroyed. In these latter points ptyalin differs from diastase, the enzyme of malt. Diastase shows a maximum action at 50° C. and is destroyed at 80° C. Effect of Reaction. — The normal reaction of saliva is slightly alkaline. Chittenden has shown, however, that ptyalin acts as well, or even better, in a perfectly neutral medium. A strong alkaline reaction retards or prevents its action. The most marked influence is exerted by acids. Free hydrochloric acid to the extent of only 0.003 per cent. (Chittenden) is sufficient to practically stop the amy- lolytic action of the enzyme, and a slight further increase in acidity not only stops the action, but also destroys the enzyme. Condition of the Starch. — It is a well-known fact that the conver- sion of starch to sugar by enzymes takes place much more rapidly with cooked starch — for example, starch paste. In the latter ma- terial sugar begins to appear in a few minutes, provided a good enzyme solution is used. With starch in a raw condition, on the contrary, it may be many minutes, or even several hours, before sugar can be detected. The longer time required for raw starch is partly explained by the fact that the starch grains are surrounded by a layer of cellulose or cellulose-like material that resists the action of ptyalin. When boiled, this layer breaks and the starch in the interior becomes exposed. In addition, the starch itself is changed during the boiling ; it takes up water, and in this hydrated condition is acted upon more rapidly by the ptyalin. The practical value of cooking vegetable foods is evident from these statements. Functions of the Saliva. — In addition to the digestive action of the saliva on starchy foods it fulfills other important functions. By moistening the food it enables us to reduce the material to a consis- tency suitable for swallowing and for manipulation by the tongue and other muscles. Moreover, the presence of mucin serves doubtless as a kind of lubricator that insures a smooth passage along the esophageal canal. Finally by dissolving dry and solid food it pro- vides a necessary step in the process of stimulating the taste nerves, and, as is described below, the activity of the taste sensations may play an important part in the secretion of the gastric juice. CHAPTER XLII. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE STOMACH. The muscular mechanisms by means of which the stomach is charged with food and in turn discharged, small portions at a time, into the duodenum have been described. The present chapter deals only with the chemical and mechanical changes in the food during its stay in the stomach and the extent to which the products of digestion are absorbed. The Gastric Glands. — The tubular glands that permeate the mucous membrane of the stomach throughout its entire extent differ in their histological structure, and therefore doubtless in their secre- tion, in different parts of the stomach. Two, sometimes three, kinds of glands are distinguished, — the pyloric, fundic (and cardiac). Those in the pyloric part of the stomach (antrum pylori) are char- acterized chiefly by the fact that in the secreting part of the tubule only one type of gland cell is found, the chief or peptic cell, while in the remainder of the stomach, but particularly in the middle or prepyloric region the glands (fundic glands) are distinguished by the presence of two types of cells, — the chief cells and the so-called cover or border cells (Fig. 283) . The third type, the cardiac glands, is found around the cardia, but its area of distribution varies in different animals, and its histological characteristics are 'not very definite.* There seems to be a general agreement that the chief cells furnish the digestive enzymes of the stomach — pepsin and rennin — and the cover cells the hydrochloric acid. From a physiological standpoint it is important to remember that the cover cells are massed, as it were, in the glands of the middle or prepyloric region of the stomach, that they are scanty in the fundus, and absent in the pyloric region. This fact is indicated to the eye by the deeper red or brownish color of the mucous membrane in the prepyloric portion. Griitznerf called especial attention to this relation, and in connection with the differences in movements of these two parts of the stomach he suggests that normally the bulk of the food toward the fundus becomes impregnated first with pepsin; then, as it is slowly moved into the prepyloric region, the acid constituent is added. The pyloric glands are said (Heidenhain) to secrete an alkaline liquid containing pepsin, and, according fco Edkins and Starling they loin, a substance which is capable of acting as a chemical excitant, Bee Haane, 'Archiv I". Anatomie," L905, I. | Grtitzner, "Archiv I', die gesammte Physiologic, " LOB, 463, 1005. 740 DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE STOMACH. 741 to the glands secreting the gastric juice (gastric secretin or gastric hormone).* Histological Changes in the Gastric Glands during Secretion. — The cells of the gastric glands, especially the so-called chief cells, show distinct changes as the result of prolonged activity. Upon preserved specimens, taken from dogs fed at intervals of twenty-four hours, Heidenhain found that in the fasting condition the chief cells were large and clear, that during the first six hours of digestion the chief cells as well as the border cells increased in size, but that in a second period, extending from the sixth to the fifteenth hour, the chief cells became gradually smaller, while the border cells remained \a m mm % m Fig. 283. — Glands of the fundus (dog): A and A1, during hunger, resting condition; B, during the first stage of digestion; C and D, the second stage of digestion, showing the diminution in the size of the "chief" or central cells. — (After Heidenhain.) large or even increased in size. After the fifteenth hour the chief cells increased in size, gradually passing back to the fasting condition (see Fig. 283). Langley * has succeeded in following the changes in a more satis- factory way by observations made directly upon the living gland. * See Starling, "Physiology of Secretion," Chicago, 1906, and Edkins, "Journal of Physiology," 1906, xxxiv., 133. t " Journal of Physiology, ' ' 3, 269, 1880. 7-12 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AXD SECRETIOX. He finds that the chief cells in the fasting stage are charged with granules, and that during digestion the granules are dissolved, dis- appearing first from the base of the cell, which then becomes filled with a non-granular material. Observations similar to those made upon other glands demonstrate that these granules represent in all probability a preliminary material from which the gastric enzymes are made during the act of secretion. The granules, therefore, are sometimes described as zymogen granules. Means of Obtaining the Gastric Secretion and its Normal Composition. — The secretion of the gastric membrane is formed in the minute glands scattered over its surface. As there is no com- mon duct, the difficulty of obtaining the secretion for analysis or experiment is considerable. This difficulty has been overcome at different times by the invention of special methods. The older methods used for obtaining normal gastric juice were very unsatisfactory. An animal was made to swallow a clean sponge to which a string wras attached so that the sponge could afterward be removed and its contents be squeezed out; or it was made to eat some indigestible material, to start the secretion of juice by mechanical stimulation; the animal was then killed at the proper time and the contents of its stomach were collected. The experiments of the older observers on gastric digestion, especially those of the Abbe Spallanzani (1729-1799), furnish most interesting reading. Spallanzani, not content with making experiments on numerous animals (frogs, birds, mammals, etc.) had the courage to carry out a great many upon himself. He swallowed foods of various kinds and in various conditions sewed in linen bags or inclosed in perforated wooden tubes which in turn were covered with linen. The bags and tubes were subsequently passed in the stools and were examined as to the amount and nature of their contents. He seems to have experienced no injury from his experiments, although normally his powers of digestion were quite feeble. As proof that the trit- urating power of the stomach is not very great he calls attention to the fact Ili.it some of the wooden tubes were made very thin, so that the slightest pressure would crush them, and yet they were voided uninjured. So also he found that cherries and grapes when swallowed whole, even if entirely ripe, were usually passed unbroken. A better method of obtaining normal juice was suggested by the famous observations of Beaumont* upon Alexis St. Martin. St. Martin, by the premature discharge of his gun, was wounded in the abdomen and stomach. On healing, a fistulous opening remained in the abdominal wall, leading into the stomach, so that the contents of the latter could be inspected. Beaumont made numerous inter- esting and most, valuable observations upon bis patient. Since that time it has become customary to make fistulous openings into the stomachs of dogs whenever it is necessary to have the normal juice * Beaumont, "The Physiology of Digestion," 1833; second edition, 1847. Por a biographical account of Beaumont, see Osier, "Journal of the American Medical issociation, " November L5, 1902. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE STOMACH. 7-13 for examination. Formerly a silver cannula was placed in the fistula, and at any time the plug closing the cannula might be re- moved and gastric juice be obtained. In some cases the esophagus has been occluded or excised so as to prevent the mixture of saliva with the gastric juice. Gastric juice may be obtained from human beings also in cases of vomiting or by means of the stomach tube, but in such cases it is necessarily more or less diluted or mixed with food and cannot be used for exact analyses, although specimens of gastric juice obtained by these methods are employed in the diagnosis and treatment of gastric troubles. From the standpoint of experimental investigation a very im- portant addition to our methods was made by Heidenhain. This observer showed that a portion of the stomach — the fundic end, for instance, or the pyloric end — might be cut away from the rest of the organ and be given an artificial opening to the exterior. By this means the secretion of an isolated fundic or pyloric sac may be obtained and examined as to its quantity and prop- erties. The method ' was subsequently improved by Pawlow, whose important contributions are referred to below. Fig. 284 gives an idea of the operation as made by Pawlow to isolate a fundic sac with its blood and nerve supply unin- jured. The normal gastric se- cretion is a thin, colorless or nearly colorless liquid with a strong acid reaction and a characteristic odor. Its spe- cific gravity varies, but it is never great, the average being about 1.002 to 1.003. Upon analysis the gastric juice is found to contain some protein, some mucin, and inorganic salts, but the essential constituents are an acid (HC1) and two or possibly three enzymes, pepsin, rennin, and lipase. Satisfactory complete analyses of the human juice have not been reported, most of the recent observers confining their attention mainly to the degree of acidity and digestive power. More complete data are published for the secretion in dogs. According to Rosemann,* the secretion in * Rosemann, "Archiv f. d. ges. Physiologie," 118, 467, 1907. Fig. 284. — To show Pawlow's operation for making an isolated fundic sac from the stomach: v, Cavity of the stomach ; s, the fundic sac, shut off from the stomach and opening at the abdominal wall, a, a; b indicates the line of sutures. — (Paw- low.) /44 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. this animal has a specific gravity of 1002 to 1004 and contains D.4277 per cent, of dry material, of which 0.1325 per cent, is ash. Analysis of the ash shows that it contains 24 per cent, of potassium, 19 per cent, of sodium, and 0.18 per cent, of calcium. The HC1 amounts to 0.55 per cent., while the total chlorine contents are more than twice that of blood. This author states, in fact, that in one animal during a secretion lasting 3\ hours about 5 gm. of chlorine were given off in the secretion, an amount about equal to that contained in the entire blood. The organic portion of the secretion, in addition to the digestive enz^mies, consists chiefly of protein. Gastric juice does not give a coagulum upon boiling, but the digestive enzymes are thereby destroyed. One of the interesting facts about this secretion is the way in which it with- stands putrefaction. It may be kept for a long time, for months even, without becoming putrid and with very little change, if any, in its digestive action or in its total acidity. This fact shows that the juice possesses antiseptic properties, and it is usually sup- posed that the presence of the free acid accounts for this quality. The Acid of Gastric Juice. — The nature of the free acid in gastric juice was formerly the subject of dispute, some claiming that the acidity is due to HC1, since this acid can be distilled off from the gas- tric juice, others contending that an organic acid, lactic acid, is present in the secretion. All recent experiments tend to prove that the acidity is clue to HC1. This fact was first demonstrated satis- factorily by the analyses of Schmidt, who showed that if, in a given specimen of gastric juice, the chlorids were all precipitated by silver nitrate and the total amount of chlorin was determined, more was found than could be held in combination by the bases present in the secretion. Evidently, some of the chlorin must have been present in combination with hydrogen as hydrochloric acid. Confirmatory evidence of one kind or another has since been obtained. Thus it has been shown that a number of color tests for free mineral acids react with the gastric juice: methyl-violet solutions are turned blue, congo-red solutions and test paper are changed from red to blue, 00 tropeolin from a yellowish to a pink red, and so on. A number of additional tests of the same general character will be found described in the laboratory handbooks.* It must be added, however, that lactic acid undoubtedly occurs, or may occur, in the stomach during digestion, [ts presence is usually explained as being due to the fer- mentalion of the carbohydrates, and it is therefore more constantly pr< 'nt in the stomachs of the herbivora. The amount of free hydrochloric acid varies according to the duration of digestion; thai is, the secretion does not possess its full acidity in the beginning owing to the facl (Heidenhain) thai in the firsl periods of digestion, * Simon, "A Manual of Clinical Diagnosis," 1904. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE STOMACH. 745 while the secretion is still scanty in amount, a portion of its acid is neutralized by the swallowed saliva, the alkaline mucus, and the alkaline secretion of the pyloric end of the stomach. It is probable that the juice as secreted has a more or less constant acidity, but after it is poured out in the stomach this acidity is not only dimin- ished by the neutralizing action of any alkalies that may be present, but, what is far more important, the free acid may be combined with the protein of the food. If the stomach contents of an animal fed on meat be examined from time to time, it may not be possible to prove the existence of free HC1 for an hour or more after the digestion has been going on, owing to the fact that it has com- bined with the protein material. In speaking of the acidity of the stomach contents, therefore, it is necessary to distinguish between the combined acid and the free acid, the two together constituting the total acidity. The acidity of the human gastric juice is usually estimated at 0.3 per cent., but during digestion it may reach (Hornborg) 0.4 to 0.5 per cent., and these figures express probably its strength as secreted. The acidity of the dog's gastric juice, according to Pawlow, lies between 0.46 and 0.56 per cent. The Origin of the HC1. — The gastric juice is the only secretion of the body that contains a free acid. The fact that the acid is a mineral acid and is present in considerable strength makes the cir- cumstance more remarkable. Attempts have been made to ascer- tain the histological elements concerned in its secretion and the nature of the chemical reaction or reactions by which it is produced. With regard to the first point it is generally believed that the border cells of the gastric tubules constitute the acid-secreting cells. This belief is founded upon the general fact that in the regions in which these cells are chiefly present — that is, the middle region of the stomach — the secretion is distinctly acid, and where they are absent or scanty in number the secretion is alkaline or less acid. In the pyloric region, for instance, these cells are lacking entirely and the secretion is alkaline. So also in the fundus the secretion does not seem to be acid, and this fact corresponds with a marked diminution or absence of the border cells. With regard to the origin of the acid it is evident that it is formed in the secreting cells, since none exists in the blood or lymph. It seems also perfectly evident that the HC1 must be formed from the chloricls of the blood. The chief chloric! is NaCl and by some means this compound is broken up: the chlorin is combined with hydrogen, and is then secreted upon the free surface of the stomach as HC1. In support of this general statement it has been shown that if the chlorids in the blood are reduced by removing them from the food for a sufficient time the secretion of gastric juice no longer contains acid. On the other hand, addition of NaBr or 746 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. KI to the food may cause the formation of some HBr and HI, together with HC1 in the gastric juice. Maly has suggested that acid ohosphates may be produced in the first instance, and then by reacting with the sodium chlorid may give hydrochloric acid, accord- ing to the formula XaH,PO, + Nad = Xa,HP04 + HC1. Other theories have been proposed, but, as a matter of fact, no explanation of the details of this reaction is satisfactory. We must be content to say that in the acid-forming cells the neutral chlorids (NaCl) are broken up with the formation of free HOI, and in all probability this reaction involves a specific metabolism on the part of these cells. The Secretory Nerves of the Gastric Glands. — Although several facts indicated to the older observers that the secretion of gastric juice is under the control of nerve fibers, we owe the actual experi- mental demonstration of this fact to Pawlow.* He demonstrated that the secretion is under the control of the nervous system and that the secretory fibers are contained in the vagus. Direct stimulation of the peripheral end of the cut vagus causes a secretion of gastric juice after a long latent period of several minutes. This long latency may be due possibly to the presence in the vagus of inhibitory fibers to the gland, which, being stimulated simultaneously with the secretory fibers, delay the action of the latter. Very striking proof of the general fact that the secretion is due to the action of vagus fibers is furnished by such experiments as these: Pawlow divided the esophagus in the neck and brought the two ends to the skin so as to make separate fistulous openings to the exterior. Under these con- ditions, when the animal ate and swallowed food it was discharged to the exterior instead of entering the stomach. The animal thus hud the enjoyment of eating without actually filling the stomach. Eating in this style forms what the author called a fictitious Hi- -ham meal (Scheinfuttcrung). It was found that it causes an abundant (low of gastric juice as long as the vagi are intact, but has no effect mi the secretion when these nerves are cut. Evidently, therefore, the sensations of taste, odor, etc, developed during the mastication and swallowing of food, set up reflexly a stimulation of secretory fibers in the vagus. Pawlow desig- nate- a aecretion produced in this way as a psychical secretion, a term which implies thai Hie reflex must be attended by conscious sensations. In favorable cases the fictitious feeding lias been continued for five or six hours and a large amount of gastric juice (70(1 C.C.) has been collected from a, fistula, although no food actually entered the stomach. It is important to note, also, that a psychical secretion, <>n<-<' started, may continue for a long time after * Sec Pawlow , "'I'll'- Work <.f i lie I Hgest ive ( Hands," translated by Thomp' Ol,. , DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE STOMACH. 747 the stimulus (the eating) has ceased. Experiments have been made upon human beings under similar conditions. Thus, Hornborg* reports the case of a boy with a stricture of the esophagus and a fistula in the stomach. Food when chewed and swallowed did not reach the stomach, but was regurgitated; it caused, nevertheless, an active psychical secretion in the empty stomach. Normal Mechanism of the Secretion of the Gastric Juice. — During a meal the gastric juice is secreted, under normal conditions, as long as the food remains in the stomach. The modern explana- tion of the origin, maintenance, and regulation of this flow of secre- tion is due chiefly to Pawlow. Contrary to a former general belief, he showed that mechanical stimulation of the gastric mucous mem- brane has no effect on the secretion of the tubules. This factor may therefore be eliminated. In an ordinary meal the secretion first started is due to the sensations of eating — that is, it is a psychical secretion. The afferent stimuli originate in the mouth and nostrils; the efferent path, the secretory fibers, is through the vagus nerve. This reflex insures the beginning at least of gastric digestion, but its effect is supplemented by a further action arising in the stomach itself. It seems that some foods contain substances designated as secretogogues, that are able to cause a secretion of gastric juice when taken into the stomach. In other foods these ready-formed secretogogues are lacking. Thus, meat extracts, meat juices, soups, etc., are particularly effective in this respect; milk and water cause less secretion. Certain common articles of food, such as bread and white of eggs, have no effect of this kind at all. If introduced into the stomach of a dog through a fistula so as not to arouse a psychical secretion, — for instance, while the dog's attention is diverted or while he is sleeping, — they cause no flow of gastric juice and are not digested. If such articles of food are eaten, however, they cause a psychical secretion, and when this has acted upon the foods some products of their digestion in turn become capable of arousing a further flow of gastric juice. The steps in the mechanism of secretion are, therefore, three: (1) The psychical secretion; (2) the secretion from secretogogues contained in the food; (3) the secretion from secretogogues contained in the prod- ucts of digestion. The manner in which the secretogogues act cannot be stated positively. Since the gastric glands possess secretory nerve fibers the first explanation to suggest itself is that the secretogogues by acting on sensory fibers in the gastric mucous membrane reflexly stimulate the secretory fibers. This explanation, however, is rendered untenable by the fact that the effect of these substances is obtained after complete severance * Hornborg, " Skandinavisches Archiv f. Physiologie," 15, 209, 1904; see also Bickel, " Verhandl. Kongr. f. innere Medizin," 23, 491. "4$ PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. of the nervous connections of the stomach. If, therefore, this so-called chemical secretion is produced by a nervous reflex the nerve centers concerned must lie in the stomach itself, the reflex must take place through the peripheral ganglion cells. Another more probable explanation has been offered. Edkins* has shown that decoctions of the pyloric mucous membrane, made by boiling in water acid or peptone solutions, when injected into the blood cause a marked secretion of gastric juice. These substances when in- jected alone into the blood cause no such effect, and decoc- tions of the mucous membrane of the fundic end of the stomach are with- out action on the gastric secretion. This author sug- gests, therefore, that the secretogogues, whether preformed in the food or formed during digestion, act upon the pyloric mu- cous membrane and form a substance which lie designates as gastrin or \f- *Tlie products intermediate between the original protein and the pep- tone are described in general a albumo e or as proteoses, according as <>no the term protein or albumin a the generic name for the original sub- tance. The term protein i generally used in English; hence, the intermedi- ate product* are more appropriately designated as proteoses. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE STOMACH. 753 sented as following in sequence. It should be stated, however, that many authors consider that even in the beginning of the digestion the protein molecule may be split into several complexes, and that some of the end-products may be formed in the very beginning of the action. All that we can state very positively is that the protein molecules undergo a series of hydrolytic cleav- ages, the end-result of which is that in place of the originally very large molecule with a weight of 5000 to 7000 there is obtained a number of much smaller and much more soluble molecules whose molecular weights are perhaps only 250 to 400 or less. It was formerly believed that pepsin was not able to split the complex protein molecule into compounds of a simpler structure than the peptone. But a number of recent authors have stated that if time enough is given the breaking up of the protein molecule may be as complete as after the action of trypsin, or after hydrolysis by acids (see Proteins in appendix). That is, along with the peptone or in place of it are found certain simpler bodies which no longer give the biuret reaction, but are precipitable by phospho- tungstic acid and for which Hofmeister proposes the general name of pep- toids. They would correspond, also, apparently, to the group of compounds designated by Fischer as peptids or polypeptids. In addition, many of the amino-acids and nitrogenous bases which constitute the final end-products of the breaking up of the protein molecule may be found.* In judging the digestive action of any given specimen of natural or artificial gastric juice it is customary to measure the rapidity with which an insoluble protein is converted into a soluble form. The method most commonly employed is that devised in Pawlow's laboratory by Mett. The Mett test is made by sucking white of egg into a thin-walled glass tube having an internal diameter of 1 to 2 mms. The egg-albumin is coagulated in the tube by immersing it for five minutes in water at 95° C. After some time the tube is cut into lengths of 10 to 15 mms. and these are used to test the digestive action or amount of pepsin. One or more of the tubes are placed in the solution to be measured and kept for ten hours at body temperature. The digestive power is measured in terms of the length in millimeters of the column of egg-albumin that is dissolved. The relative amounts of pepsin in solutions compared in this way are determined by the law of Schiitz, according to which the digestive power is proportional to the square root of the amount of pepsin. If in two specimens of gastric juice the number of millimeters of egg albumin digested was in one case two and in the other three, the digestive powers of the two solutions would be as the squares of the numbers, as 4 to 9. The Rennin Enzyme (Rennet, Chymosin). — The property possessed by the mucous membrane of the calf's stomach of curdling milk has been known from remote times, and has been utilized in the manufacture of cheese and curds. This action takes place with remarkable rapidity under favorable conditions, a large mass of milk * See Hofmeister, " Ergebnisse der Physiologie," vol. i., part r., 79(5, 1902. 48 754 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. setting to a firm coagulum within a very brief time. It has been shown that this effect is due to an enzyme — remain or rennet. The rennin. like the pepsin, is supposed to be formed in the chief cells of the gastric tubules and to be present in the glands in a zymogen form, the prorennin or proehymosin, which after secretion is con- verted to the active enzyme. This conversion takes place very readily under the influence of acid. Rennin (or its zymogen) may be obtained easily from the mucous membrane of the stomach (with the exception of the pyloric end) by extracting with glycerin or water or by digesting with dilute acid. Good extracts of rennin cause the milk to clot with great rapidity at a temperature of 40° C; the milk (cows' milk), if undisturbed, sets at first into a solid clot, which afterward shrinks and presses out a clear, yellowish liquid — the whey. With human milk the curd is much less firm, and takes the form of loose flocculi. The whole process resembles much the clotting of blood. The rapidity of clotting is said to vary inversely as the amount of rennin, or, in other words, the product of the amount of rennin and the time necessary for clotting is a constant. The curdling of the milk involves two apparently independent proc- esses : First, the rennin acts upon the casein of the milk and converts it into a substance known as paracasein. The paracasein then reacts with the lime salts of the milk, forming an insoluble calcium salt, which constitutes the curd or coagulum. According to this view, the enzyme does not cause clotting directly. What takes place when the casein is changed to paracasein is not understood. Ham- marsten originally regarded the change as a cleavage process, but this view has not been supported. Others have supposed that a transformation or rearrangement of molecular structure occurs. Indeed, the differences in properties between casein and paracasein are not great, the most marked difference being that the calcium salts of the latter are insoluble. If soluble calcium salts are removed from milk by the addition of oxalate solutions it does not curdle upon the addition of rennin. Addition of lime salts restores this property. Jt should be added that casein is also precipitated from milk by the addition of an excess of acid. The curdling of sour milk in the formation of bonnyclabber is a well-known illustration of this fact. When milk stands for sonic time the action of bacteria upon the milk- sugar leads to the formation of lactic acid, and when this acid reaches a certain concentration it causes the precipitation of the ■ an. So far as our positive knowledge goes, t-M(' action of rennin is confined to milk. Casein is the chief protein constituent of milk, ;inCHCHNH,COOH. * Abderhalden, "Zeitschrift f. physiol. Chemie," 44, 17, 1905. Consult for general description of the digestion of proteins. '6(3 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION". Leucin or aminocaproic acid: £§3^CHCH,CHNH,COOH. As stated above, this compound was one of the first end-products of protein hydrolysis that was recognized. It may be obtained readily in crystalline form. CHNH,COOH Aspartic or aminosuccinic acid : CH,COOH. r,, + • • ., nxr /CHNH,COOH Glutammic acid: t/H3' pu COOH BENZENE OR AROMATIC SERIES. Tyrosin (para-oxyphenylaminopropionic acid) : C6H4OH . CH2 . CHNH2- COOH. This substance was also among the first recognized prod- ucts of protein hydrolysis It occurs early in the process of pan- creatic digestion, and is easily obtained in crystalline form from the digested mixture. It is especially interesting because of the presence of a benzene nucleus, thus giving proof that the benzene grouping occurs normally in the protein molecule. Phenylalanin (phenylaminopropionic acid) : C6H5CH,CHNH,COOH. This benzene derivative is, according to Abderhalden, split off from the protein with difficulty by the action of trypsin, although readily produced by acid hydrolysis. PYRROL AND INDOL SERIES. CH, — CH2 I I Prolin or a-pyrrolidin carboxylic acid: CH2 CHCOOH. This substance, NH discovered first by Fischer among the products of acid hydrolysis of proteins, lias since been shown to occur in tryptic digestion. Like the glycin and phenylalanin, it is produced with difficulty by trypsin acting alone, but more readily if the tryptic action follows upon previous peptic digestion, as is the case in the body. Tryptophan (indolaminopropionic acid) : This substance has long been recog- nized among the products of tryptic digestion by the reddish-violet color (Tiedemann and Gmelin, 1X2<>) observed upon the addition of chlorin or bromin. Its chemical st rucl ure was determined by Hopkins and Colo (1001). According to Ellinger,* tryptophan is an indol- amino-propionic acid of the formula (' . ( "1 1( '< )( )1I( 'lLNIl.,. c,h4<(Vh NH When fed to dogs it causes the appearance of kynurenic acid (C,„H7.\< >,) in t he urine. It is interesting as showing the existence of an indol grouping in t lie protein molecule. II. 'I'm: DlAMINO-BODIES (HEXON BASES). Lysin fo-#-diaminocaproic acid): C'UI IMN2< >-. or < ' 1 1 :. \ 1 1 2 (( ' II ;,) .,0 J I N J I2- COOH. Arginill (guanidin «-aininovalerianic acid): C|,IInN.,()2 or NH(!NII2NH- CH,(CH3)aCHNH,COOH. Hi tidin: CgHjNgOj (imidazolaminopropionic acid?). The Significance of Tryptic Digestion. It was formerly supposed thai the objeel of peptic and tryptic digestion is to con- bhe insoluble and non-dialyzable proteins into the simpler, more soluble, and more diffusible peptones and proteoses, In this * Ellinger. " Zeitschrifl f. physiol. Chemie," 43, '■'^.',, 1904. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE INTESTINES. 767 way absorption of protein material was explained. This, view, however, is not sufficient. On the one hand, it has not been possible to prove conclusively that peptones or proteoses are found in the blood; on the other hand, a better knowledge of the processes of tryptic or of peptic-tryptic digestion has shown that the hydrolysis does not stop at the peptone stage ; the protein molecule is split into a number of simpler crystalline substances, the various amino- bodies. At present different views exist as to the extent of this latter process. Some believe that the protein molecule is entirely broken down into its so-called end-products, and that in order to serve its nutritive function these products or some of them must be synthetically combined again during or after absorption. This view is supported, moreover, by the discovery of the existence of the enzyme erepsin (see below) in the intestinal mucosa. The action of this latter enzyme is exerted especially upon the albumoses and pep- tones, breaking them down into the amino-acids, so that apparently whatever peptone or albumose may escape the final action of the trypsin before absorption is likely to be acted upon by the erepsin before reaching the blood.* Another interesting view is that sug- gested by Abderhalden.f According to this author, the hydrolysis of the protein by pepsin and trypsin (and perhaps by erepsin) is not complete. Many amino-bodies, such as tyrosin, leucin, arginin, etc., are split off from the protein molecule, but there remains behind what one may call a nucleus of the original molecule, which serves as the starting point for a synthesis. This nucleus is a substance or a number of substances intermediate between the peptone and the simpler end-products, and is spoken of as a peptid or peptoid (see Appendix) . Since its structure is unknown and it is probably not a simple body, it is designated as a polypeptid. Abderhalden has shown that in tryptic digestion such substances are formed — that is, substances which are not peptones, since they no longer give the biuret reaction, but which have a certain complexity of structure, since upon hydrolysis with acids they split into a number of mon- amino- and diamino-bodies. A schema of peptic-tryptic digestion from this standpoint may be given as follows: Native protein. Peptone. Polypeptid. Tyrosin, leucin, glutaminic acid, aspartic acid, a-pyrrolidin carboxylic acid, tryptophan, etc. Arginin, lysin, histidin. * Vernon ("Journal of Physiology," 30, 330, 1904) believes that the pancreatic secretion contains two proteolytic enzymes — trypsin proper, which converts the proteins to peptones, and pancreatic erepsin, which breaks up the peptones into the simpler end-products, the amino-bodies. f Abderhalden, loc. cit. 76S PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. From either of the points of view presented it may be suggested that the value of this more or less complete splitting of the protein of the food lies in the possibility that thereby the body is able to construct its own peculiar type of protein. Many different kinds of proteins are taken as food ami many of them if introduced directly into the blood act as foreign material incapable of nourishing the tissues. If these proteins are broken down more or less completely during digestion the tissue cells may reconstruct from the pieces a form of protein adaptable to their needs, and more or less characteristic fur that particular organism. This general point of view is gaining ground in recent years and has obtained much support from the fart that an animal may be nourished properly on a diet in which the protein of the food is entirely replaced by the split products of a complete pancreatic digestion (see p. 857). Action of the Diastatic Enzyme (Amylase) of the Pan- creatic Secretion. — This enzyme is found in the secretion of the pancreas or it may be extracted from the gland. Its action upon starch}' foods is closely similar to or identical with that of ptyalin. It causes an hydrolysis of the starch with the production finally of maltose and aehroodextrin. Before absorption these substances are further acted upon by the maltase of the intestinal secretion and converted to dextrose. The starchy food that escapes digestion in the mouth and stomach becomes mixed with tins enzyme in the duodenum, and from that time until it reaches the end of the large intestine conditions are favorable for its conversion to maltose and dextrin. Most of this digestion is probably completed, under normal conditions, before the contents of the intestinal canal reach the ileo- cecal valve. Action of the Lipolytic Enzyme (Lipase, Steapsin). — The importance of the pancreatic secretion in the digestion of fats was firsi clearly stated by Bernard (1849). We know now that this secre- tion contains an active enzyme capable of hydrolyzing or saponifying the neutral fats. These latter bodies are chemically esters of the trihydric alcohol glycerin. When hydrolyzed they break up into glycerin and the constituent fatty acid. The action of lipase may be represented, therefore, by the following reaction, in the case of palmitLn : I ii C II .COO), -I- 3Ha0 - C,H,(0]l)3 -i- 3(C,8H8,COOH) j'.iliiiiiin. ' ilj rii in. I'.-ilmii io acid. When lipase from any source is added to neutral oils its splitting action is readily recognized by (lie development of an acid reaction flue to the formation of the fatty acid. 11* a, bit of fresh pancreas • \f the sugars: In fact, it probably goes on for the period of four or five hours, during which an ordinary meal is making its progress from pylorus to ileocecal valve. During this period the entire quantity of blood in the body is passed through the mesenteric arteries over and over again, and it is probable that even in the portal vein the quantity of sugar at any one moment rises but little above the normal level, and this small excess is held back by the liver colls, so that the systemic circulation is protected from becoming hyperglycemic. So far as the carbohydrates escape absorption as sugar they are liable to undergo acid fermentation from the bacteria always present in the intestine. As the result of this fermentation there may be produced acetic acid, lactic acid, butyric acid, succinic acid, carbon dioxid, alcohol, hydrogen, etc. This fermentation probably occurs to some extent in the small intestines under normal conditions. Macfadyen,* in the case already referred to, found that the contents of the intestine at the ileocecal valve contained acid equivalent to that of a 0.1 per cent, solution of acetic acid. Under less normal conditions, such as excess of sugars in the diet or deficient absorp- tion, the large production of acids may lead to irritation of the intes- tines,— diarrhea, etc. Absorption of Fats. — Numerous theories have been held in regard to the mode of absorption of fats. It has been supposed that the emulsified (neutral) fat is ingested directly by the epithelial cells, that the fat droplets enter between the epithelial cells in the so-called cement substance, that the fat droplets are ingested by leucocytes that lie between the epithelial cells, or lastly that the fat is first split into fatty acid and glycerin and is absorbed by the epithelial cells in these forms. The tendency of recent work is to favor this last view. During digestion the epithelial cells contain fat droplets without doubt, but it seems probable that these droplets are formed in situ by a synthesis of the absorbed glycerin and fatty acids. The border of the cell is said to be free from fat globules, — a fact which would indicate that the neutral hit is not mechanically ingested as oil drops. But, granting thai the fat is absorbed in solution, as fatty acids and glycerin, the mechanism of absorption remains unexplained. It is known lh:il the bile as well as the pancrent LC juice plays an important part in the process. The pancreatic juice furnishes i he lipase, (lie bile furnishes the bile salts (glycocholate and taurocholate of sodium) which aid the lipase in splitting the neutral fat, and moreover aid greatl) the absorption of the split fats. This latter function is due probably to the fad thai the bile (bile salts) dissolves (he fat ty acids * Macfadyen, Nencki, .-nil Sieber, loc. dt. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE INTESTINES. I (Q readily* and thus brings them into contact, in soluble form, with the epithelial cells. When the bile is drained off from the intestine by a fistula of the gall-bladder or duct, a large proportion of the fatty foods escapes absorption and appears in the feces. Direct observation shows that the fat after passing the epithelial lin- ing and entering the stroma of the villus is taken up by the lymphatic vessels, the so-called lacteals. This fact is beautifully demonstrated by the mere appearance of the lymphatics of the mesentery after a meal containing fats. These vessels are injected with milky chyle during the period of absorption so that their entire course is revealed. The chyle on microscopical exami- nation is found to contain fat in the form of an extremely fine emulsion. In this form it is carried to the thoracic duct and thence to the venous circulation. For hours after a meal the blood contains this chyle fat. If a specimen of blood is taken during this time and centrifugalized in the usual way, the chyle fat may be collected at the top in the form of a cream. It is an easy matter to insert a cannula into the thoracic duct at the point at which it opens into the subclavian and jugular veins and thus collect the entire amount of fat absorbed from the intestines by way of the lacteals. Experiments of this kind show that, after deducting the amount of fat that escapes absorption and is lost in the feces, the amount that may be recovered from the thoracic duct is less than that taken in the food. It seems probable, therefore, that some of the fat is absorbed directly by the blood-vessels of the villi, probably in the form of fatty acids or soaps. The portion thus absorbed enters the portal vein and passes through the liver before reaching the general circulation. The liver holds back more or less of the fat taking this route, as it is found that during absorption the liver cells show an accumulation of fat droplets in their interior .f The amount of fat that may be absorbed from the intestines varies with the nature of the fat. Experiments show that the more fluid fats, such as olive oil, are absorbed more com- pletely,— that is, less is lost in the feces than in the case of the more solid fats. Comparative experiments have given such results as the following: Olive oil, — absorption, 97.7 per cent. ; goose and pork fat, 97.5 per cent.; mutton fat, 90 to 92.5 per cent.; spermaceti, 15 per cent. The amount of fat that may be lost in the feces varies also with other conditions. If, for instance, an excess is taken with the food, or if the bile flow is diminished or suppressed, the percentage in the feces is increased. The usual amount of fat allowed as a maximum in dietaries is from 100 to 120 gms. daily. Absorption of Proteins. — Most of the experimental work on record shows that the digested proteins are absorbed by the blood- vessels of the villi, although after excessive feeding of protein a *See Moore and Rockwood, "Journal of Physiology," 21, 58, 1897; also Moore and Parker, " Proceedings, Roval Society," London, 58, 64, 1901. t See Frank, " Archiv f. Physiologie, " 1892, 497, and 1894, 297. 776 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. portion may be taken up also in the lymphatics.* This accepted belief rests upon two facts: First (Schmidt-Mulheim), if the thoracic duct (and right lymphatic duct I is ligated, so as to shut off the lym- phatic circulation, an animal will absorb and metabolize the usual amount of protein as is indicated by the urea excreted during the pe- riod. Second (Munk), if a fistula of the thoracic duct is established and the total lymph How from the intestines is collected during the period of absorption after a diet of protein, it is found that there is no increase in the quantity of the lymph or in its protein contents. The form in which protein is absorbed and circulates in the blood is not satisfactorily determined. Under normal conditions the protein food is digested by the successive actions of pepsin, trypsin, and probably erepsin. During this digestion peptones and proteoses are formed and may be absorbed as such, or they may be further broken down by trypsin and erepsin to the amino-bodies, leucin, tyrosin, arginin, etc.. and the intermediate compounds, the poly- peptids or peptoids (see p. 767), and be absorbed in the form of these split products. Some observers claim to have found peptones or proteoses as a normal constituent of the blood, but this claim has not been satisfactorily established. Others have shown the presence of traces of the amino-acids,f but much uncertainty exists as i'i the precise form in which the protein nourishment for the body exists normally in the blood. Several possibilities have been sug- gested. It is conceivable that the peptones or the more simple split products may be synthesized in the wall of the intestine or in the liver to the proteins of the blood, the serum-albumin or globulin; it i- possible that many of the end-products of the digestive splitting may be further oxidized and converted to urea in the liver and only a fractional part be really synthesized into the proteins of the body, or it is possible that the absorbed protein exists in the blood in some special form not as yet recognized. Perhaps the most importanl fad to be emphasized in this connection is the dis- covery thai animals may lie nourished when fed only with tin; split products of protein, thai is to Bay, with the products of a complete pancreatic digestion (p. Nf)7). It is evident that in such cases the body must take some of these split products and build 1 hem up again to t he protein form. The prevailing bypol n- i- thai 1 his synthesis takes place in the walls of the intestine ami that the body protein thus reconstructed constitutes a part of the proteins of the blood. It must, be borne in mind, however, that this, hypothesis is far from having been demonst rated. Atten- * Sec Mendel, "American Journal of Physiology," 2, 137. L899. f For reference . see Howell, "American Journal of Physiology," 1906, 17, 273. DIGESTION AND ABSORPTION IN THE INTESTINES. 777 tion should also be directed to the fact that many forms of protein may be absorbed apparently without previous digestion. This fact has been demonstrated for isolated loops of the small intestine and also for parts of the large intestine. It is, moreover, borne out by the medical practice of giving enemata into the rectum when the conditions are such that the patient can not be fed in the normal way. That absorption and utilization of the protein take place under such conditions is shown not only by the improved nutritive condition of the individual, but also by the increased output of nitrogen in the urine. This phenomenon occurs in parts of the intestinal canal in which normally no proteolytic enzymes occur, so that the whole process must be referred to an activity of the cells composing the walls of the intestine. There seems at present little grounds for a satisfactory explanation of the absorption of proteins, with or without digestion, by a direct application of the known laws of osmosis, diffusion, and imbibition. Examination of the contents of the small intestine at its junction with the large shows that under normal conditions most of the protein has been ab- sorbed before reaching this point. The process is continued in the large intestine, modified somewhat by bacterial action, and the amount that finally escapes absorption and appears in the feces varies, in perfectly normal individuals, with the character of the protein eaten. According to Munk,* the easily digestible animal foods — such as milk, eggs, and meat — are absorbed to the extent of 97 to 99 per cent., while with vegetable foods the utilization is less complete. This difference is not due, however, to any peculiarity of the vegetable proteins; it is probably an incidental result of the presence of the indigestible cellulose found in our vegetable foods. It is stated that from 17 to 30 per cent, of the protein may be lost in the feces if the vegetable food is in such form as not to be attacked readily by the digestive secretions. Digestion and Absorption in the Large Intestine. — Observa- tions upon the secretions of the large intestine have been made upon human beings in cases of anus praeternaturalis, in which the lower portion of the intestine was practically isolated, and also upon lower animals, in which an artificial anus was established at the end of the small intestine. These observations all indicate that the secretion of the large intestine, while it contains much mucus and shows an alkaline reaction, is not characterized by the presence of distinctive enzymes. When the contents of the small intestine pass the valve they still contain a certain amount of unabsorbed food material. As was stated in the chapter on the movements of the intestine, this material remains a long time in the large intestine, and since it contains the digestive enzymes received in the duodenum the * See Munk, "Ergebnisse der Physiologic," vol. i., part i., 1902, article, "Resorption," for literature and discussion. 7ile and which is an excretion to l»' removed; secondly, they facilitalc greatly 'lie splitting and the absorption of fats in the intestine. It. ♦ Friedmann, " Hofmeister'e Beitrage, " '■'>, I, 1902. t See Simon, ".Johns Hopkins Hospital Bulletin," l.r>, .'{(if), 1004. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE LIVER AXD SPLEEX. 787 is an undoubted fact that when bile is shut off from the intestine the absorption of fats is very much diminisned, and it has been shown that this action of the bile in fat absorption is due chiefly to the presence of the bile-acids, and in the same way the known acti- vating influence of bile upon the activity of pancreatic lipase has been traced to the bile-acids. The bile-acids, the taurocholate, at least, possess the property of precipitating proteins in acid solu- tions. This property probably explains the fact that the acid chyme as it passes into the duodenum is precipitated by coming into contact with the bile, a fact which has long been known, although its physiological significance is not clear. Cholesterin. — Cholesterin is a non-nitrogenous substance of the formula C27H460. (See p. 79.) It is a constant constituent of the bile, although it occurs in variable quantities. Cholesterin is very widely distributed in the body, being found especially in the white matter (medullary substance) of nerve fibers. It seems, moreover, to be a constant constituent of all animal and plant cells. It is assumed that cholesterin is not formed in the liver, but that it is eliminated by the liver cells from the blood, which collects it from the various tissues of the body. According to Naunyn, however, the cholesterin is not secreted by the liver cells proper, but is added to the secretion while in the bile pas- sages— the gall-ducts and gall-bladder. That it is an excretion- is indicated by the fact that it is eliminated in the feces. Choles- terin is insoluble in water or in dilute saline liquids, and is held in solution in the bile by means of the bile-acids. We must regard it as a waste product of cell life, formed probably in minute quantities, and excreted mainly through the liver. It is partly eliminated through the skin, in the sebaceous and sweat secretions, and in the milk. Lecithin, Fats, and Nucleo-albumin. — Lecithin, C42H84- XP09, is a compound of glycerophosphoric acid with fatty acid radicals (stearic, oleic, or palmitic) and a nitrogenous base, cholin (see p. 79). When hydrolyzed by boiling with alkali it splits up into these three substances. It is found generally as such, or in combination, in all cells, and evidently plays some as yet unknown part in cell metabolism. It occurs in largest quantity in the white matter of the nervous system. In the liver it occurs to a considerable extent both as lecithin and in a more complex combination with a carbohydrate residue, a compound designated as jecorin. So far as it is found in the bile, it represents possibly a waste product derived from the liver or from the body at large. Little is known of its precise physiological significance. According to Hewlett and others it may serve to activate the lipase of the pancreatic secretion. 788 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. The special importance, if any, of the small proportion of fats and fatty acids in the bile is unknown. The ropy, mucilaginous character of bile is due to the presence of a body formed in the bile- ducts and gall-bladder. This substance was formerly designated as mucin, but it is now known that in ox bile at least it is not a true mucin, but a nucleo-albumin (see appendix). Hammarsten reports that in human bile some true mucin is found. Outside the fact that it makes the bile viscous, this constituent is not known to possess any especial physiological significance. The Secretion of the Bile. — Numerous experiments have been made to ascertain whether or not the secretion of bile is controlled by a special set of secretory fibers. The secretion itself is continuous, but varies in amount under different conditions. These conditions may be controlled experimentally in part. It has been shown, for example, that stimulation of the spinal cord or splanchnic nerve diminishes the flow of bile, while section of the splanchnic branches may cause an increased flow. These and similar actions are ex- plained, however, by their effect on the blood-flow through the liver. The splanchnics carry vasomotor nerves to the liver, and section or stimulation of these nerves will therefore alter the circulation in the organ. Since the secretion increases when the blood-flow is increased and vice versa, it is believed that in this case no special secretory nerve fibers exist. The metabolic processes in the liver cells which produce the secretion probably go on at all times, but they are increased when the blood-flow is increased. We may believe, there- fore, that the quantity of the bile secretion varies with the quantity and composition of the blood flowing through the liver. On the physiological and pharmacological side efforts have been made to discover what substances stimulate especially the formation of bile. Such suhstances are designated as cholagogues. The therapeutical agents capable of giving this action are still a subject of controversy. On the physiological side the following facts are accepted : Any agent thai cause- .hi hemolysis of red corpuscles increases the flow of bile, or the same effect is produced if a solution of hemoglobin is injected directly into the blood. This result is in harmony with the views already stated regarding the significance of the bile pigments as an lory product of hemoglobin. The cholagogue whose action is most distinct and prolonged is bile itself. When fed or injected directly into the circulation bile causes an undoubted increase in the ecretion. This effect is due both to the bile acids and bile pigments. Since the bile acids have a hemolytic effect on n^\ corpuscles, it might at firsl be assumed that their action as cholagogues is due indirectly to this circumstance. The net ion of the bile acids is, however, much more pronounced than that of other hemolytic agents, and it eeme certain, therefore, that they exert a specific effect on the PHYSIOLOGY OF THE LIVER AND SPLEEN. 789 liver cells. Lastly, there is evidence that the secretin, whose action upon the pancreatic secretion has been described, exerts a similar effect upon the secretion of bile. Statements differ somewhat in regard to the extent of this action, but it seems to be certain that, when acids (0.5 per cent. HC1) are injected into the duodenum or upper part of the jejunum, the secretion of bile is increased; and, since this effect takes place when the nervous connections are severed, the effect, as in the case of the pancreatic secretion, is explained by as- suming that the acid converts prosecretin to secretin, and this latter after absorption into the blood acts upon the liver cells.* A similar effect may be obtained by injecting secretin directly into the blood. Since during a meal the stomach normally ejects acid chyme into the duodenum, the importance of this secretin reaction in adapting the secretion of bile to the period of digestion is evident. The Ejection of Bile into the Duodenum — Function of the Gall-bladder. — Although the bile is formed more or less continu- ously, it enters the duodenum periodically during the time of digestion. The secretion during the intervening periods is prevented from enter- ing the duodenum apparently by the fact that the opening of the common bile-duct is closed by a sphincter. The secretion, therefore, backs up into the gall-bladder. According to Bruns,f no bile appears in the duodenum as long as the stomach is empty. When, how- ever, a meal is taken, the ejection of the chyme into the duodenum is followed by an ejection of bile. J It would seem, therefore, that each gush of chyme into the duodenum excites, probably by reflex action, a contraction of the gall-bladder, and an inhibition of the sphincter closing the opening into the intestine. An interesting application of this fact has been made in surgical practice. After operations upon the gall-bladder trouble is experienced at times owing to the failure of the fistulous opening to heal, so that there is constant oozing of gall. It is found that frequent feeding of the patient facilitates the per- manent closure of the fistula, because apparently the sphincter is kept inhibited and the pressure in the gall-bladder is lowered. The substances in the chyme that are responsible for the stim- ulation have been investigated by Bruns. He finds that acids, alkalies, and starches are ineffective, and concludes that the reflex is clue to the proteins and fats or some of the products of their digestion. The gall-bladder has a muscular coat of plain muscle, and records made of its contractions show that the force exerted is quite small. According to Freese,§ the maximal contraction * See Falloise, quoted in Maly's " Jahres-bericht der Thier-chemie," 33, 611, 1904. t " Archives des sciences biologiques," 7, 87, 1899. j See also Klodnizki, quoted from Maly's "Jahres-bericht der Thier- chemie," 33, 617, 1904. § "Johns Hopkins Hospital Bulletin," June, 1905. 790 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. does not exceed that necessary to overcome the hydrostatic pressure of a column of water 220 nuns, in height, — a force, therefore, which is about equivalent to the secretion pressure of bile as determined by Heidenhain. The innervation of the gall-bladder and gall-ducts has been studied especially by Doyon.J It would seem, from the experiments made by this author together with later experiments reported by others, f that the bladder receives both motor and in- 10 8 6 4 Z 30' JO' 30' JO' JO' JO'- JO' JO' 30' JO' 30' 30' 30' JO' JO' / v 1 , \ I \ / j \ 8 6, 2 1 \ I \ .-"- -., t ~~~ \ ', \ 8 e z / / j \ ,- '' \ / \ / \ —f — ' \s --.. FiK. 2S7. — Curves showing the velocity of secretion of bile into the duodenum on (1) a dial "I r 1 1 i 1 k , uppermost curve; (-) a diet of meat, middle curve; (3) a diet of bread, [owe i curve. The divisions on the ab ci B represent intervals of thirty minutes; the figures on the ordinates represent the volume of secretion in cubic centimeters. — (firiins.) hibitory fibers by way of the splanchnic nerves. These fibers emerge from the spinal cord in the roots of the sixth thoracic to the first lumbar spinal nerve, and pass to the celiac plexus by way of the splanchnic nerves. Motor fibers may occur also in the vagi. Sensory liber- capable of causing :i icilex constriction or dilatation of the bladder are found in both the vagus and splanchnic nerves. Stim- ulation of the central end of the cu1 splanchnic causes a dilatation of the bladder (reflex Btimulation of the inhibitory libers), while stimulation of the centra] end of the vagus causes a contraction of the bladder and a dilatation (inhibition) of the sphincter muscle Doyon, "Archive* de phy iologie," L894, p. I'd. Bainbridge and Dale, 'Journal or Physiology," L905, \\\iii., L38. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE LIVER AXD SPLEEN. 791 at the opening of the common duct into the intestine. Since these last movements are the ones that occur during normal digestion, it is probable that the afferent fibers from the duodenum, which are concerned in this reflex, run in the vagus. Effect of Complete Occlusion of the Bile-duct. — When the flow of bile is prevented by ligation of the bile-duct, or when this duct is occluded by pathological changes the bile eventually gets into the blood, producing a condition of jaundice (icterus). There has been much discussion as to whether the bile is absorbed directly into the blood from the liver cells or the liver lymph-spaces, or whether it is carried to the blood by way of the lymph-vessels and thoracic duct.* Experimental evidence points to both possibili- ties. The increased pressure in the bile system leads possibly to a rupture of the delicate bile capillaries, and the bile thus escapes into the lymph-spaces. From these spaces it may be absorbed directly by the blood-vessels of the liver, or it may be carried off in the lymph-stream toward the thoracic duct. General Physiological Importance of Bile. — The physiological value of bile has been referred to in speaking of its several constitu- ents. Bile is of importance as an excretion in that it removes from the body waste products of metabolism, such as cholesterin, lecithin, and bile pigments. With reference to the pigments, there is evidence to show that a part at least may be reabsorbed while passing through the intestine, and be used again in some way in the body. The bile acids represent end-products of metabolism involving the proteins of the liver cells, but they are undoubtedly reabsorbed in part, and can not be regarded merely as excreta. As a digestive secretion, the most important function attributed to the bile is the part it takes in the digestion and absorption of fats. It accelerates greatly the action of the lipase of pancreatic juice in splitting the fats to fatty acids and glycerin, and it aids materially in the absorption of the products of this hydrolysis. A number of observers have shown that when a permanent biliary fistula is made, and the bile is thus prevented from reaching the intestinal canal, a large proportion of the fat of the food escapes absorption and is found in the feces. This action of the bile may be referred directly to the fact that the bile acids serve as a solvent for the fats and fatty acids. It was formerly believed that bile is also of great importance in restraining the processes of putre- faction in the intestine. It was asserted that bile is an efficient antiseptic, and that this property comes into use normally in prevent- ing excessive putrefaction. Bacteriological experiments made by a number of observers have shown, however, that bile itself has very feeble antiseptic properties, as is indicated by the fact that it putrefies * See Mendel and Underbill for literature, "American Journal of Phys- iology," 1905, xiv., 252. 792 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. readily. The free bile acids and eholalic acid do have a direct retard- ins: effect upon putrefactions outside the body; but this action is not very pronounced, and has not been demonstrated satisfactorily for bile itself. It seems to be generally true that in cases of biliary fistula the feces have a very fetid odor when meat and fat are taken in the food. But the increased putrefaction in these cases may possibly be an indirect result of the withdrawal of bile. It has been suggested, for instance, that the deficient absorption of fat that follows upon the removal of the bile results in the protein and carbohydrate material becoming coated with an insoluble layer of fat, so that the penetration of the digestive enzymes is retarded and greater opportunity is given for the action of bacteria. We may conclude, therefore, that, while there does not seem to be sufficient warrant at present for believing that the bile exerts a direct antiseptic action upon the intestinal contents, nevertheless its presence limits in some way the extent of putrefaction. Glycogen. — One of the most important functions of the liver is the formation of glycogen. This substance was found in the liver in 1857 by Claude Bernard, and is one of several brilliant discoveries made by him. Glycogen has the formula (C6H10O5)n, which is also the general formula given to vegetable starch ; glycogen is therefore frequently spoken of as " animal starch." It gives, however, a port- wine-red color with iodin solutions, instead of the familiar deep blue of vegetable starch, and this reaction serves to detect glycogen not only in its solutions, but also in the liver cells. Glycogen is readily soluble in water, and the solutions have a characteristic opalescent appearance. Like starch, glycogen is acted upon by ptyalin and other cliastatic enzymes, and the end-products are apparently the same — namely, maltose, or maltose and some dextrin, or else dex- trose, depending upon the enzyme used. Under the influence of acids it may be hydrolyzed at once to dextrose.* Occurrence of Glycogen in the Liver. — Glycogen can be detected in the liver cells microscopically. If the liver of a dog is removed twelve or fourteen hours after a hearty meal, hardened in alcohol, and sectioned, the liver cells are found to contain clumps of clear material which give (lie iodin reaction for glycogen. Kven when distinct aggregations of the glycogen can not be made out, its presence in the cells is shown by the red reaction with iodin. By this simple method one can demonstrate the important fact that the amount of glycogen in the liver increases after meals and decreases again during the fasting hours, and if the fast is sufficiently prolonged it may disappear altogether. This fact is, however, shown more satisfactorily by quantitative determinations, by chemical means, * The extensive literature of glycogen is collected und reviewed by Cre- mer in the " Ergebni e der Phyeiologie, " vol. i, part i, 1902; and by Pfliiger, " \n-hiv f. die gesammte Physiologic," 90, I, L903. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE LIVER AND SPLEEN. 793 of the total glycogen present. The amount of glycogen in the liver is quite variable, being influenced by such conditions as the character and amount of the food, muscular exercise, body tem- perature, drugs, etc. From determinations made upon various animals it may be said that the average amount lies between 1 .5 and 4 per cent, of the weight of the liver. But this amount may be in- creased greatly by feeding upon a diet largely made up of carbohy- drates. It is said that in the dog the total amount of liver glycogen may be raised to 17 per cent., and in the rabbit to 27 per cent., by this means, while it is estimated for man (Neumeister) that the quan- tity may be increased to at least 10 per cent. It is usually believed that glycogen exists as such in the liver cells, being deposited in the substance of the cytoplasm. Reasons have been brought forward to show that this is not strictly true, and that the glycogen is prob- ably held in some sort of weak chemical combination. It has been shown, for instance, that although glycogen is easily soluble in cold water, it can not be extracted readily from the liver cells by this agent. One must use hot water, salts of the heavy metals, and other similar agents that may be supposed to break up the combination in which the glycogen exists. For practical purposes, however, we may speak of the glycogen as lying free in the liver-cells, just as we speak of hemoglobin existing as such in the red corpuscles, although it is probably held in some sort of combination. Origin of Glycogen. — To understand clearly the views held as to the origin of liver glycogen, it is necessary to describe briefly the effect of the different foodstuffs upon its formation. Effect of Carbohydrates on the Amount of Glycogen. — The amount of glycogen in the liver is affected very quickly by the quantity of car- bohydrates in the food. If the carbohydrates are given in excess, the supply of glycogen may be increased largely beyond the average amount present, as has been stated above. Investigation of the differ- ent sugars has shown that dextrose, levulose, saccharose (cane-sugar), and maltose are unquestionably direct glycogen-formers, — that is, glycogen is formed directly from them or from the products into which they are converted during digestion. The bulk of our car- bohydrate food reaches the liver as dextrose, or as dextrose and levu- lose, and these forms of sugar may be converted into glycogen in the liver cells by a simple process of dehydration, such as may be repre- sented in substance by the formula C6H1206 — H20 = C6H10O5. There is no doubt that both dextrose and levulose increase markedly the amount of glycogen in the liver; and, since cane-sugar is inverted in the intestine before absorption, it also must be a true glycogen- former, — a fact that has been abundantly demonstrated by direct experiment. Lusk* has shown, however, that, if cane-sugar is in- * Voit, "Zeitschrift f. Biologie," 28, 285, 1891. 794 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. jeeted under the skin, it has a very feeble effect in the way of increas- ing the amount of glycogen in the liver, since under these conditions it is probably absorbed into the blood without undergoing inversion. Experiments -with subcutaneous injection of lactose gave similar results, and it is generally believed that the liver cells can not convert the double sugars to glycogen, at least not readily; hence the value of the hydrolysis of these sugars in the alimentary canal before absorption. We may assume, therefore, that dextrose, levulose, and galactose are the true glycogen-formers that occur normally in the blood, and that the disaccharids (cane-sugar, milk-sugar, etc.) and the polysaccharids (starches) are true glycogen-formers to the ex- tent that they are converted into dextrose, levulose, or galactose. Effect of Protein on (rh/cogen Formation. — In his first studies upon glycogen Bernard asserted that it may be formed from protein material. Since that time there have been much discussion and experimentation upon this point. The usual view is that protein must be counted among the true glycogen-formers in the sense that some of the material of the protein molecule is directly converted to glycogen. The protein in digestion undergoes, it will be remem- bered, a splitting process, the limits of which are not definitely settled. It i- assumed, however, that the nitrogenous split products are acted upon in the liver, the nitrogen being converted first to an ammonia compound and then to urea, while the non-nitrogenous residue is converted to sugar by a synthetic process. Positive results have been obtained showing that some, at least, of the amino acids, such as glycin, alanin, and aspartic acid, may be converted to sugar in the body. Experimentally observers find for the warm-blooded animals, at least, that feeding with proteins, even in the case of those proteins, such as casein, that contain no carbohydrate grouping, causes an increased production of glycogen.* The conclusion to be drawn from these experiments engthened by clinical experience upon human beings suffer- ing from diabetes. In severe forms of this disease the carbo- hydrate material of the food escapes oxidation in the bod}' and i.^ secreted unchanged in the urine. If under these conditions the individual is given an exclusively protein diet, sugar still continues to appear in the urine, and it would seem that this sugar can only arise from the protein food. In the similar condition of severe glycosuria that may be pro- duced by the use of phloridzin it has been shown that the animal continues to excrete Ugar even when U-<\ on protein alone or when starved. Under such conditions the amount of dextrose in the urine bears a definite ratio to the amount of nitrogen excreted D:N::3.65: I (Lusk), which would indicate that both arise from * Bee Stookey, Ami rican Journal of Physiology," '■), L38, L903. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE LIVER AND SPLEEN. 795 the breaking down of the protein molecule. On this supposition 58.4 per cent, of the protein may be converted to sugar. So also the fact that during prolonged starvation, lasting for forty or even ninety days, the blood retains a practically constant composition in sugar indicates that this material is being formed from either the protein or fat supply of the body. Other considerations tend to exclude the fat, and we are, therefore, led to the belief that the protein can give rise to sugar in the body. If this change is part of the normal metabolism of the body it would make protein a gly- cogen-former, since the sugar formed from the protein may, of course, be converted to glycogen. Whether or not all proteins yield gly- cogen or sugar in the body is not entirely determined. Some authors have thought that only those proteins that contain a carbohydrate residue have this property; but, as stated above, casein and other proteins that do not possess this grouping seem also to increase the glycogen supply when fed alone. Effect of Fats upon Glycogen Formation. — A large number of substances have been found by some observers to increase the store of glycogen in the liver. In some of these cases at least it is evident that the substance is not a direct glycogen-former in the sense that the material is itself converted to glycogen. It may increase the supply of liver glycogen in some indirect way, — for example, by diminishing the consumption of glycogen in the body. The most important substance in this connection from a practical standpoint is fat. Whether or not the body can convert fats into sugar or glycogen is a question about which at present there is much difference of opinion, and much evidence might be cited on each side. Cremer, however, has furnished apparent proof that glycerin acts as a direct glycogen or sugar-former. When fed, especially in the diabetic condition, it causes an increase in the sugar which can not be explained as a result of protein metabolism. Since in the body neutral fats are normally split into glycerin and fatty acid, the fact that glycerin can be converted to sugar seems to carry with it the admission that fats may contribute directly to sugar production. Whether the synthesis of sugar (or glycogen) from glycerin is, so to speak, a normal process or occurs only under especial condi- tions, cannot be decided at present. Since, however, the glycerin radicle constitutes but a small fraction of the fat molecule, the quantitative importance of a change of this kind cannot be very great under any circumstances. The Function of Glycogen — Glycogenic Theory. — The meaning of the formation of glycogen in the liver has been, and still is, the subject of discussion. The view advanced first by Bernard is perhaps most generally accepted. According to Bernard, glycogen forms a temporary reserve supply of carbo- 796 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. hydrate material that is laid up in the liver during digestion and is gradually made use of in the intervals between meals. During digestion the carbohydrate food is absorbed into the blood of the portal system as dextrose or as dextrose, levulose, and galactose. If these sugars passed through the liver unchanged, the contents of the systemic blood in sugar would be increased perceptibly. It is now known that when the percentage of sugar in the blood rises above a certain low limit a condition of hyperglycemia prevails, and the excess is excreted through the kidney and is lost. But as the blood from the digestive organs pusses through the liver the excess of sugar is abstracted by the liver cells, is dehydrated to make glycogen, and is retained in the cells in this form for a short period. An objection has been made to this part of the glycogenic hypothesis by Paw on the ground that if all the carbohydrates of a meal were absorbed into the blood as free sugar, a condition of hyperglycemia and glycosuria must evidently result. We know that glycosuria does occur when the carbohydrates are eaten in excess (alimentary glj-cosuria) for this very reason. But within what we may call the normal limits of a carbohydrate diet it seems most probable that the contents of the portal vein never rise much above the usual level, since the carbohydrate is absorbed slowly during a period of four to five hours, and during this period a very large amount of blood must flow through the intestines, as much perhaps in five hours as 180 to 190 liters, if one may apply to man the results of Burton- Opit z, obtained for the dog, namely, a flow of 31 cc. per minute for each 100 gms. of intestine. From time to time the glycogen of the liver is reconverted into sugar (dextrose) and is given off to the blood. By this means the percentage of sugar in the systemic blood is kept nearly constant (0.1 to 0.2 per cent.) and within limits best adapted to the use of the tissues. The great importance of the formation of glycogen and the consequent conservation of the sugar supply of the tissues is evident when we consider the nutritive value of carbohydrate food. Carbohydrates form the bulk of our usual diet, and the proper regulation of the supply to the tissues is, there- fore, of vital importance in the maintenance of a normal, healthy condition. The second part of this theory, which holds that the glycogen is reconverted to dextrose, is supported by observations upon livers removed from the body. It has been found that shortly after the removal of the liver the supply of glycogen begins to dis- appear and a Corresponding increase ill dextrose occurs. Within a comparatively short time all the glycogen is gone and only dextrose is found. It is for this reason that in t lie estimation of glycogen in the liver it ifi necessary to mince the organ and to throw it, into boiling water a- quickly as possible, sincp by this means the liver cells are PHYSIOLOGY OF ^HE LIVER AND SPLEEN. 797 killed and the conversion of the glycogen is stopped. How the gly- cogen is changed to dextrose by the liver is a matter not fully ex- plained. According to most authors, the conversion is due to an enzyme produced in the liver. Extracts of liver, as of some other tissues, yield a diastatic enzyme that changes glycogen to dextrose.* It is probable, therefore, that the normal conversion of glycogen to dextrose is effected by a special enzyme produced in the liver cells. In this description of the origin and meaning of the liver glycogen reference has been made only to the glycogen derived directly from digested carbohydrates. The glycogen derived from protein foods, once it is formed in the liver, has, of course, the same functions to fulfil. It is converted into sugar, and eventually is oxidized in the tissues. For the sake of completeness it may be well to add that some of the sugar of the blood formed from the glycogen, when an excess is eaten beyond the energy needs of the tissues, may be converted into fat in the adipose tissues instead of being burnt, and in this way it may be retained in the body as a reserve supply of food of a more stable character. Glycogen in the Muscles and other Tissues. — The history of glycogen is not complete without some reference to its occurrence in the muscles. Glycogen is, in fact, found in various places in the body, and is widely distributed throughout the animal kingdom. It occurs, for example, in leucocytes, in the placenta, in the rapidly growing tissues of the embryo, and in considerable abundance in the oyster and other molluscs. But in our bodies and in those of the mammals generally the most significant occurrence of glycogen, outside the liver, is in the voluntary muscles, of which glycogen forms a normal constituent. It has been estimated that the percentage of glycogen in resting muscle varies from 0.5 to 0.9 per cent., and that in the musculature of the whole body there may be contained an amount of glycogen equal to that in the liver itself. Muscular tissue, as well as liver tissue, has a glycogenetic function — that is, it is cap- able of laying up a supply of glycogen from the sugar brought to it by the blood. The glycogenetic function of muscle has been demonstrated directly by Kulz,f who has shown that an isolated muscle irrigated with an artificial supply of blood to which dextrose is added is capable of changing the dextrose to glycogen, as shown by the increase in the latter substance in the muscle after irriga- tion. Muscle glycogen is to be looked upon as a temporary and local reserve supply of material; so that, while we have in the liver a large general depot for the temporary storage of glycogen for the use of the body at large, the muscular tissue, which, considering its bulk, is the most active tissue of the body from the standpoint * Tebb, "Journal of Physiology," 22, 423, 1897-98. t "Zeitschrift f. Biologie," 72, 237, 1890. 79S PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. of energy production, is also capable of laying up in the form of glycogen any excess of sugar brought to it. The fact that glycogen occurs so widely in the rapidly growing cells of embryos indicates that this glycogenetic function may at times be exercised by any tissue. Conditions Affecting the Supply of Glycogen in Muscle and Liver. — In accordance with the view given above of the general value of glycogen — namely, that it is a temporary reserve supply of carbohydrate material that may be rapidly converted to sugar and oxidized with the liberation of energy — it is found that the supply of glycogen is greatly affected by conditions calling for increased metabolism in the body. Muscular exercise quickly exhausts the supply of muscle and liver glycogen, provided it is not renewed by new food. Observations on isolated muscles have shown definitely that the local supply of glycogen is diminished when the muscle is made to contract (see p. 66). In a starving animal glycogen finally disappears, except perhaps in traces, but this disappearance occurs much sooner if the animal is made to use its muscles at the same time. It has been shown also by Morat and Dufourt that if a muscle has been made to contract vigorously it takes up much more sugar from an artificial supply of blood sent through it than a similar muscle which has been resting ; on the other hand, it has been found that if the nerve of one leg is cut so as to paralyze the muscles of that side of the body, the amount of glycogen is greater in these muscles than in those of the other leg that have been contracting meantime and using up their gly- cogen. The further history of glycogen is considered in the section on Nutrition. Formation of Urea in the Liver. The nitrogen contained in the protein material of our food is finally eliminated, mainly in the form of urea. It has been definitely proved that the urea is not formed in the kidneys, the organs that eliminate it. It has long been considered a matter of the greatesl importance to ascertain in what organ or tissues urea is formed. Investigations have gone so far as to demonstrate thai it arises in part at least in the liver; hence the property of forming urea must be added to the other important func- tions of the liver cell. Schroder* performed a number of experi- ments in which the liver was taken from a freshly killed dog and irrigated through its blood-vessels with a supplj of blood obtained from another dog. If the supply of blood was taken from a fasting animal, then circulating it through the isolated liver was not followed by any increase in the amount of urea contained in it. If, on the contrary, the blood was obtained from a well-fed dogj the amount " Arcbiv f. experimentelle Pathologie unci Pharmakologie," 15, 364, L882, and I'.i, 373, L885. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE LIVER AND SPLEEN. 799 of urea contained in it was distinctly increased by passing it through the liver, thus indicating that the blood of an animal after digestion contains something that the liver can convert to urea. It is to be noted, moreover, that this power is not possessed by all the organs, since blood from well-fed animals showed no increase in urea after being circulated through an isolated kidney or muscle. As further proof of the urea-forming power of the liver Schroder found that if ammonium carbonate was added to the blood circulating through the liver — to that from the fasting as well as from the well-nourished animal — a very decided increase in the urea was always obtained. It follows from the last experiment that the liver cells are able to convert carbonate of ammonium into urea. The reaction may be expressed by the equation (NH4)2C03— 2H20 = CON2H4. Schon- dorff * in some later work showed that if the blood of a fasting dog is irrigated through the hind legs of a well-nourished animal, no increase in urea in the blood can be detected; but if the blood, after irrigation through the hind legs, is subsequently passed through the liver, a marked increase in urea results. Obviously, the blood in this experiment derives something from the tissues of the leg which the tissues themselves cannot convert to urea, but which the liver cells can. Finally, in some remarkable experiments upon dogs made by four investigators (Hahn, Massen, Nencki, and Pawlow), which are described more fully in the next chapter, it was shown that when the liver is practically destroyed there is a distinct diminution in the urea of the urine. In birds uric acid takes the place of urea as the main nitrogenous excretion of the body, and Minkowski has shown that in them removal of the liver is followed by an important diminution in the amount of uric acid excreted. From experiments such as these it is safe to conclude that urea is formed in the liver and is then given to the blood and excreted by the kidney. In treating of the physiological history of urea an account will be given of the views proposed with regard to the antecedent substance or substances from which the liver produces urea. Physiology of the Spleen. — Much has been said and written about the spleen, but we are yet in the dark as to the distinctive function or functions of this organ. The few facts that are known may be stated briefly without going into the details of theories that have been offered at one time or another. The older experimenters demonstrated that this organ may be removed from the body without serious injury to the animal. An increase in the size of the lymph- glands and of the bone-marrow has been stated to occur after ex- tirpation; but this is denied by others, and, whether true or not, it gives but little clue to the normal functions of the spleen. Some observers t find that the removal of the spleen causes a marked * Pfluger's "Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 54, 420, 1893. f Laudenbach, " Centralblatt fur Physiologie," 9, 1, 1895. 800 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. diminution in the number of red corpuscles and the quantity of hemoglobin. They infer, therefore, that the spleen is normally concerned in some way in the formation of red corpuscles. Others, however, report with equal positiveness that removal of the spleen has no effect upon the number of red corpuscles or upon the power of the animal to regenerate its corpuscles after hemorrhage.* The most definite facts known about the spleen are in connection with its movements. It has been shown that there is a slow expansion and contraction of the organ synchronous with the digestion periods. After a meal the spleen begins to increase in size, reaching a maximum at about the fifth hour, and then slowly returns to its previous size. This movement, the meaning of which is not known, is probably due to a slow vasodilatation, together, perhaps, with a relaxation of the tonic contraction of the musculature of the trabecular. In addition to this slow movement, Royf has shown that there is a rhythmical contraction and relaxation of the organ, occurring in cats and dogs at intervals of about one minute. Roy supposes that these con- tractions are effected through the intrinsic musculature of the organ, — that is, the plain muscle tissue present in the capsule and trabecular, — and he believes that the contractions serve to keep up a circulation through the spleen and to make its vascular supply more or less independent of variations in general arterial pressure. The fact that there is a special local arrangement for maintaining its cir- culation makes the spleen unique among the organs of the body, but no light is thrown upon the nature of the function fulfilled. The spleen is supplied richly with motor nerve fibers which when stimu- lated either directly or refiexly cause the organ to diminish in volume. According to 8chaefer,J these fibers are contained in the splanchnic nerves, which carry also inhibitory fibers whose stimu- lation produces a dilatation of the spleen. The chemical composition of the spleen is complicated, but sug- gestive. Its mineral constituents are characterized by a large percent age of iron, which seems to be present as an organic compound of some kind. Analysis shows also the presence of a number of fatty acids, fats, cholesterin, and, what is perhaps more noteworthy, a number of nitrogenous extractives belonging to the group of purin bases, .such as xanthin, hypoxanthin, adenin, guanin, and uric acid. The presence of these bodies seems to indicate that active metabolic change,-- of some kind occur in the spleen. As to the theories of the splenic functions, the following may be mentioned: (1) The spleen been supposed fco give rise to new red corpuscles. This it un- doubtedly does during fetal life and shortly after birth, and in some animals throughout life, but then; is no reliable evidence that the ; Paton, Gulland, and Fowler, "Journal of Physiology," 28, 83, L902. Journal of Physiology," 3, 203, L881. :|: Ibid., 20, L, L896. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE LIVER AND SPLEEN. 801 function is retained in adult life in man or in most of the mammals. The presence of a large amount of iron in organic combination suggests, however, that the spleen may play a part in the prepara- tion of new hemoglobin, or in the perservation of the iron set free by the death of the red corpuscles. This suggestion is strengthened by the fact that after extirpation of the spleen there is a distinct increase in the daily loss of iron from the body, in dogs an increase from 11 to 18 or 29 mgm.* (2) It has been supposed to be an organ for the destruction of red corpuscles. This view is founded chiefly on microscopical evidence, according to which certain large ameboid cells in the spleen ingest and destroy the old red corpuscles, and partly upon the fact that the spleen tissue seems to be rich in an iron-containing compound. This theory cannot be considered at present as satisfactorily demon- started. (3) It has been suggested that the spleen is concerned in the production of uric acid. This substance is found in the spleen, as stated above, and it was shown by Horbaczewsky that the spleen contains substances from which uric acid or xanthin may readily be formed by the action of the spleen-tissue itself. More recent investigations f have shown that the spleen, like the liver and some other organs, contains special enzymes (adenase, guanase, and xanthin oxydase), by whose action the split products of the nucleins may be converted to uric acid, and it is probable, therefore, that this latter substance is constantly formed in the spleen. (4) Lastly, a theory has been supported by Schiff and Herzen, according to which the spleen produces something (an enzyme) which, when carried in the blood to the pancreas, acts upon the trypsinogen con- tained in this gland, converting it into trypsin. This view has been corroborated by a number of observers, but it is difficult at present to decide whether such an action occurs normally during digestion. As already stated, the general testimony at present indicates that the pancreatic juice when secreted contains its trypsin in inactive form. It is activated only after reaching the duodenum under the influence of the enterokinase. * Grossenbacher and Asher, " Zentralblatt f. Physiol," No. 12, 1908. f Consult Jones and Austrian, "Zeitschrift f. physiol. Chem.," 1906, xlviii., 110. 51 CHAPTER XLV. THE KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. Structure of the Kidney. — The kidney is a compound tubular gland. The uriniferous tubules composing it may be roughly separated into a secreting part comprising the capsule, convoluted tubes, and loop of Henle, and a collecting part, the so-called straight or collecting tube, the epithelium of which is assumed not to have any secretory function. Within the secreting part the epithe- lium differs greatly in character in different regions; its peculiarities may be referred to briefly here so far as they seem to have a physio- Fiir. 288. — Portions of the various divisions of the uriniferous tubules drawn from ■ections of human kidney: A, Malpighian body: x, squamou epithelium lining the cap> sule and reflected over the glomerulus; y, z, afferent and efferent ve els "f the tuft; >■., nuclei of capillarie ; n, con tricted neck marking pa age "i cap ule into convoluted tu« bule; /-'. proximal convoluted tubule; C. irregular tubule; /' and F, piral tubules; /.', aacending limb of Henle' loop; 0, traighf collecting tubule. {Pieraol.) logical bearing, although for a complete description reference must be made to works on histology. The arrangemenl of the glandular epithelium in the capsule with reference to the bloods essels of I he glomerulus is worl hy of special attention. If will be remembered that each Malpighian corpuscle con- ■ if t wo principal parts, a I nil of blood-vessels, the glomerulus, and an enveloping expansion of the uriniferous tubule, the capsule. The glomerulus is an interesting structure (see Fig. 288, A I. If consists 802 KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 803 of a small afferent artery which after entering the glomerulus, breaks up into a number of capillaries. These capillaries, although twisted together, do not anastomose, and they unite to form a single efferent vein of a smaller diameter than the afferent artery.' The whole structure, therefore, is not an ordinary capillary area, but a rete mirabile, and the physical factors are such that within the capil- laries of the rete there must be a greatly diminished velocity of the blood-stream, owing to the great increase in the width of the stream bed, and a high blood-pressure as compared with ordinary capil- laries. Surrounding this glomerulus is the double-walled capsule. One wall of the capsule is closely adherent to the capillaries of the glomerulus; it not only covers the structure closely, but dips into the interior between the small lobules into which the glomerulus is divided. This layer of the capsule is composed of flattened, endo- thelial-like cells, the glomerular epithelium, to which great impor- tance is attached in the formation of the secretion. It will be no- ticed that between the interior of the blood-vessels of the glomerulus and the cavity of the capsule, which is the beginning of the urin- iferous tubule, there are interposed only two very thin layers, — ■ namely, the epithelium of the capillary wall and the glomerular epithelium. The apparatus would seem to afford most favorable conditions for filtration of the liquid parts of the blood. The epi- thelium clothing the convoluted portions of the tubule, including under this designation the so-called irregular and spiral portions and the loop of Henle, is of a character quite different from that of the glomerular epithelium (Fig. 288, B, C, D, E, F, G). The cells, speaking generally, are cuboidal or cylindrical, protoplasmic, and granular in appearance; on the side toward the basement mem- brane they often show a peculiar striation, while on the lumen side the extreme periphery presents a compact border which in some cases shows a cilia-like striation. These cells have the general appearance of an active secretory epithelium, and one theory of urinary secretion attributes this function to them. The Secretion of Urine. — The kidneys receive a rich supply of nerve fibers, but most histologists have been unable to trace any connection between these fibers and the epithelial cells of the kidney tubules. The majority of purely physiological experiments upon direct stimulation of the nerves going to the kidney are adverse to the theory of secretory fibers, the marked effects obtained in these ex- periments being all explicable by the changes produced in the blood- flow through the organ. Two general theories of urinary secretion have been proposed. Ludwig held that the urine is formed by the simple physical processes of filtration and diffusion. In the glom- eruli the conditions are most favorable to filtration, and he sup- S04 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION* AXD SECRETION. posed that in these structures water filtered through from the blood, carrying with it not only the inorganic salts, but also the specific elements (urea, etc.) of the secretion. There was thus formed at the beginning of the uriniferous tubules a complete but diluted urine, and in the subsequent passage of this liquid along the con- voluted tubes it became concentrated by diffusion with the more concentrated lymph surrounding the outside of the tubules. Bowman's theory of urinary secretion, which has since been vigorously supported and extended by Heidenhain, was based orig- inally mainly on histological grounds. It assumes that in the glomeruli water and inorganic salts are produced, while the urea and related bodies are eliminated through the activity of the epi- thelial cells in the convoluted tubes. The first of these theories (Ludwig) is sometimes spoken of as the mechanical theory, since as originally proposed it attempted to explain the formation and composition of the urine by reference only to the physical forces of filtration and diffusion. Adherents of this view in recent years have modified it, however, to the extent that the absorption supposed to take place in the convoluted tubules is designated as a selective absorption, or selective diffusion, the characteristics of which depend upon unknown peculiarities of struc- ture in the epithelial cell, so that it is no longer a purely mechanical theory. The difference between the Ludwig and the Bowman theories may be stated briefly in this way. The former assumes that in the glomerulus all of the constituents of the urine are produced from the blood, probably by nitration, and that the function of the epithelium lining the convoluted tubules is absorptive, like the epithelium of the intestines, and not secretory. The Bowman view as formulated by Heidenhain teaches that the glomerular epithe- lium forms the water and salts of the urine by an act of secretion, the ultimate chemistry or physics of which is not known. The theory asserts that the epithelial cells participate actively in the process of secretion and do not serve simply as a passive membrane. The cells of the convoluted tubules are also secretory, their special activity being limited mainly to the organic constituents, urea, etc., although, iri this respect, — namely, in the precise distinction be- tween the secretory products of the glomerular epithelium and those of the convoluted tubules, — the theory is not very explicit. Much interest and a large literature have been stimulated by controver- sy- based on these theories, and to-day the facts accumulated are qo1 such as to demonstrate conclusively one view or the other, although, on the whole, perhaps, it may be said that the majority of physiologists adhere to the more conservative view of Bowman- Heidenhain bo the extent at least of recognizing that the physical laws of filt ration, diffusion, and imbibition, so far as they are known, KIDNBY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 805 do not suffice for a satisfactory explanation of the facts.* As in other similar cases, our knowledge of the physical structure and chemical properties of the walls of the living cells is still very de- ficient, and it seems necessary to designate these activities by the indefinite term secretion. Function of the Glomerulus. — As stated above, the structure of the glomerulus is peculiar and suggestive of a special adaptation. Ludwig's theory looks upon it as a filter, the pressure of the blood in the glomerular capillaries driving the water and salts through the endothelium of the capillaries and the glomerular epithe- lium into the cavity of the urinary tubule. If we consider only the water and assume that the membranes traversed are freely permeable to its molecules, then it is evident that, upon this theory, the quantity of urine formed will depend upon the filtration pres- sure, and that this filtration pressure can be expressed by the formula F=P — p, in which P represents the blood-pressure in the glom- erular capillaries and p the pressure of the urine in the capsular end of the uriniferous tubules. Some of the interesting facts de- veloped by experiment may be presented in connection with this formula. According to the mechanical theory, the amount of urine formed should vary directly with P and inversely with p. The factor P may be increased in two general ways: First, by those changes which raise general arterial pressure and therefore the pressure in the renal arteries, — such changes, for instance, as are brought about by an increased force of heart beat or a large vaso- constriction. Second, by obstructing or occluding the renal veins. Experiments have been made along these lines. With regard to the first possibility it has been found in general, although not invar- iably, that raising arterial pressure increases the quantity of urine if the means used are such as may be assumed to raise the pressure in the glomerular capillaries. The reverse experiment, however, of raising P by blocking the venous outflow fails entirely to support the theory. When the renal veins are compressed the capillary pressure in the glomeruli must be increased, and, if the veins are blocked entirely, we may suppose that the capillary pressure is raised to the level of that of the renal arteries. In such experiments, however, the flow of urine is di- minished instead of being increased, and indeed may be stopped altogether when the veins are completely blocked. The adherents of the Ludwig theory have attempted to explain this unfavorable result by assuming that the swollen interlobular veins press upon and block the uriniferous tubules. According to the antagonistic theory of Heidenhain, blocking the veins suppresses the secretory activity of the glomerular epithelium by depriving it of oxygen and * For discussion and literature see Magnus, "Munchener med. Wochen- schrift," 1906, Nos. 28 and 29. 806 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AXD SECRETION. the chance for removal of CO.,, — that is, by producing local as- phyxia. The latter explanation seems the simpler of the two, and it is very strongly supported by the opposite experiment of clamping the renal artery. When this is done the blood-flow through the kidney ceases and the secretion of urine also stops, as would be expected. But when after a few minutes' closure the artery is un- damped, the secretion is not restored with the return of the cir- culation. On the contrary, a long time (as much as an hour or more) may elapse before the secretion begins. This fact is quite in harmony with the Heidenhain theory, since complete removal of their blood supply might well result in a long-continued injury to the delicate epithelial cells. On the mechanical theory, however, we should expect a contrary result. Injury to the cells should be followed by greater permeability and an increased filtration, as is found to be the case with the production of lymph. These two experiments, blocking the renal artery and the renal vein, seem at present to dis- credit the filtration theory and to support the secretion theory. If we accept this latter theory it may be asked how it agrees with the experiments mentioned above upon the variations in capillary pressure brought about otherwise than by obstructing the venous outflow. Heidenhain has emphasized the fact that all of these ex- periments involve not only a variation in capillary pressure, but also in the blood-flow, and that it is open to us to suppose that the effect upon the secretion of urine is dependent upon the rate of flow rather than upon the capillary pressure. If we adopt this expla- nation we are led again to the secretion hypothesis. Mere rate of flow should not influence filtration, but might affect secretion, since it would alter the . composition of the blood flowing through the glomeruli and also the amount of oxygen and carbon dioxid. An important fact, which seems at first sight to show a direct influence of pressure, is that when general arterial pressure falls below a cer- tain point, about 40 nuns, of mercury, the secretion of urine ceases altogether. Such a condition may be brought about by surgical shock, by hemorrhage, or by section of the spinal cord in the cer- vical or thoracic region. But here again the great vascular dila- tation causing this fall of pressure! is associated with a feeble cir- culation, and the effect upon I he kidney secretion may well be due to this latter factor. In addition to varying the factor P in the formula, given above, it is possible also to increase the factor />. Normally the pressure of i he urine in the capsule must be very low, owing to the fact that i he secretion drains away as rapidly as if is formed. If the ureter LS OCCluded, however, the pressure of the urine will increase, and the filtration pressure P — p will diminish. When this experiment is performed and the pressure in the ureter is measured by a, nianojn- KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 807 eter, it is found to rise to 50 or 60 mms. of mercury and then to remain stationary. This fact might be explained b}^ supposing that when p = P the secretion stops on account of the failure of the filtration pressure. Little weight, however, can be given to this argument, since it is quite possible that under these condi- tions the urine may still continue to form, but be reabsorbed under the high tension reached. The experiment simply serves to show the secretion pressure of the urine, and the fact that this pressure rises as high as 50 to 60 mms. mercury, while the capillary pressure is probably somewhat lower, would rather serve as an argument against the nitration theory. Moreover, experiments show * that when a certain moderate resistance is established in the ureters (p = 10 cms. H20) the flow of urine is actually increased instead of falling off, a fact entirely opposed to the mechanical theory, but explicable on the secretion theory on the assumption that the resistance acts as a stimulus. Function of the Convoluted Tubule. — By the term convoluted tubule is meant here the entire stretch from the glomerulus to the straight tubules. Its epithelium varies in character; its cells are distinguished in general, as contrasted with the glomerular epithe- lium, by a relatively large amount of granular protoplasm. The question of interest at present in regard to this epithelium is whether it is secretor}r or absorptive. The original view of Ludwig that diffusion takes place in these tubules between the urine and the blood (lymph) in accordance with simple physical laws and that by this action alone the dilute urine is brought to its normal concen- tration must be abandoned. The mere fact that the urine may be more concentrated in certain constituents than the blood is suffi- cient evidence that other factors must co-operate. Those who be- lieve that the main function of the tubules is absorptive are obliged to regard this process as physiological, as a selective absorption depending upon the living structure and properties of the epithelial cells. The kind of evidence upon which this view is based is some- what indirect; a single example may suffice. Cuslmy statesf that if certain diuretics — for example, sodium chlorid and sodium sul- phate— are injected simultaneously into the blood and in such amounts that an equal number of the anions (CI and S04) are pres- ent, the quantities that are excreted in the urine during the next hour or two follow different curves and vary independently of their concentration in the plasma. While this independence might be referred to a specific secretory action, the author finds a simpler explanation in variations in absorption, the epithelium of the con- voluted tubule, like that of the intestine, absorbing the sulphate with more difficulty. On the other side, the facts that have been * Brodie and Cullis, "Journal of Physiology, " 1906, xxxiv., 224. t "Journal of Physiology," 27, 429, 1902. SOS PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. urged in favor of the secretory hypothesis are more numerous and varied, but none is entirely convincing. Some of these facts are as follows: (1) It is stated that if the ureters are ligated in birds the urates -will be found deposited in the uriniferous tubules, but never at the capsular end. (2) Heidenhain has given proof that the convoluted tubules are capable of excreting indigo-carmin after this substance is injected into the blood. His experiment consisted essentially in injecting the material into the blood, after dividing the cord so as to reduce the rapidity of secretion. After a certain interval the kidney was removed and irrigated with alcohol to pre- cipitate the indigo-carmin in situ in the organ. Microscopical ex- amination showed that after this treatment the granules of the indigo-carmin are found in the convoluted tubules, but not in the capsules around the glomeruli. (3) Microchemical reactions indicate that the iron secreted from the kidney as well as the uric acid is given off through the epithelium of the convoluted tubules. (4) Several observers (Van der Stricht, Disse, Trambasti, Gur- witsch*) have described microscopical appearances in the cells lining the tubules indicative of an active secretion. They picture the formation of vesicles in the cells and appearances which indi- cate the discharge of these vesicles into the cavity of the tubules. (5) Nussbaum made use of the fact that in the frog the glomeruli are supplied by branches of the renal artery, while the rest of the tubes are supplied by the renal portal vein. He stated that if the renal artery is ligated the glomeruli are deprived completely of blood, and that as a result the flow of urine ceases. If under these conditions urea is injected into the circulation, it is excreted together with some water, thus proving the secretory activity of the tubules with regard to urea. These results, although denied at one time, have recently been confirmed and extended.! (6) Dreser has shown that the acidity of the urine is due to an action of the epithelium of the tubules. If an acid indicator, such as acid t'nchsin, is injected into the dorsal lymph-sac of a frog, and an hour or so later the kidneys are examined, it will l>e found that the convoluted tubules are colored red, while the capsular end is colorless, indicating that the secretion at the latter point has an alkaline react ion. The experiment shows that t he acid substances in the urine arc produced in the convoluted tubules. The sim- plesl explanation is that they are formed by a, secretory activity of the epil hclial cells. (7) St udics of the gaseous exchanges in the kidney during diuresis} and during the; glycosuria caused by phlorhizinf tend to support the secretion hypothesis to the Gurwil ch, "Archiv I'. die gesammte Phyeiolocie, " 91, 71, L902. I Bainbridge and Beddard, "Journal of Physiology, moo, xxxiv. (Proc. I'liv iol. Soc ); al o Cullis., ibid., p. '_'.">(). ; Barcroft and Brodie. "Journal of Physiology," 1906, xxxiii., 52. I' •■■ y, Brodie, and Siam, ibid., 1903, xxix., l<>7. KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 809 extent that they prove an increased metabolism during func- tional activity. (8) The action of diuretics (see below). On the whole, it must be admitted that the weight of evidence is in favor of the Bowman-Heidenhain theory of secretion, and it remains for future investigations to explain more definitely what is meant by the obscure term "secretory activity." Under pathological conditions it has been shown satisfactorily that the albumin and sugar which may be present in the urine are secreted or eliminated at the glomerular end of the tubule. Action of Diuretics. — An important side of the theories of secretion of urine is their application to the action of diuretics. Water; various soluble substances, such as salts, urea, and dextrose; and certain special drugs, such as caffein or digitalis, exert a diuretic action on the kidneys. Much experimental work has been done to ascertain whether the action of these substances can be explained mechanically by their influence on the blood-flow or the blood- pressure in the kidney capillaries, or whether it is necessary to fall back upon a specific stimulating effect exerted by them upon the epithelial cells of the tubules. Adherents of the original Ludwig theory are forced to explain their action by the effect they pro- duce upon the pressure in the kidney capillaries, and, indeed, it has been shown with reference to the saline diuretics that their effect upon the secretion is in proportion to the osmotic pressure they exert. It has been suggested, therefore, that the action of these diuretics lies in the fact that they attract water from the tis- tues into the blood and thus cause a condition of hydremic plethora. But whether the elimination of this excess of water is due to filtra- tion or to an active secretion by the glomerular epithelium is a question that revives the discussion that has been presented briefly above. Most observers find that the vascular changes in the kid- ney, particularly after the administration of caffein and digitalis, do not explain satisfactorily the phenomenon of diuresis, and al- though it is necessary to admit that the diuretics, or some of them, act in part by the changes which they cause in the circulation in the kidney, it is not possible to demonstrate that all the phenomena under this head can be thus explained. The bulk of the work published indicates that some at least of the known diuretics act as stimulants to the secreting cells. In the case of the inorganic salts it may be said (Magnus) that there is for each salt a "secretion threshold." An increase in concentration above this level leads to the elimination of the excess of salt and an increased secretion of water. The Blood-flow through the Kidneys. — It will be inferred from the discussion above that, other conditions remaining the same, the secretion of the kidney varies with the quantity of blood flowing through it. It is, therefore, important to refer briefly to the nature 810 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. and especially the regulation of the blood-flow through this organ, although the same subject is referred to in connection with the general description of vasomotor regulation (see Circulation). It has been shown by Landergren* and Tigerstedt that the kidney is a very vascular organ, at least when it is in strong functional activ- ity such as may be produced by the action of diuretics. They esti- mate that in a minute's time, under the action of diuretics, an amount of blood flows through the kidney equal to the weight of the organ; this is an amount from four to nineteen times as great as occurs in the average supply of the other organs in the systemic circulation. Taking both kidneys into account, their figures show that (in strong diuresis) 5.6 per cent, of the total quantity of blood sent out of the left heart in a minute may pass through the kidneys, although the combined weight of these organs makes only 0.56 per cent, of that of the body (see table p. 453). The nature of the supply of vasomotor nerves to the kidney and the conditions which bring them into activity are fairly well known, owing to the useful invention of the oncometer by Roy. This in- stalment is, in principle, a plethysmograph especially modified for use upon the kidney of the living animal. It is a kidney-shaped box of thin brass made in two parts, hinged at the back, and with a clasp in front to hold them together. In the interior of the box thin peritoneal membrane is so fastened to each half that a layer of olive oil may be placed between it and the brass walls. There is thus formed in each half a soft pad of oil upon which the kidney rests. When the kidney, freed as far as possible from fat and sur- rounding connective tissue, but with the blood-vessels and nerves entering at the hilus entirely uninjured, is laid in one-half of the on- cometer, and the other half is shut down upon it and tightly fas- tened, the organ is surrounded by oil in a box which is liquid-tight at every point except one, from which a tube is led off to some suitable recorder such as a tambour. Under these conditions every increase in the volume of the kidney causes a proportional outflow of oil from the oncometer, which is measured by the recorder, and every diminution in volume is accompanied by a reverse change. At the same time (he flow of urine during these changes can be determined by inserting a cannula info the ureter and measuring directly the outflow of urine. By this and other means it has been shown that the kidney receives a rich supply of vasoconstrictor nerve fiber- thai reach it between and around Hie entering blood- Is. rhese fillers emerge from the spinal cord chiefly in the lower thoracic spinal nerves (tenth to thirteenth in the dog), pass through the sympathetic system, and reach the organ as postgan- glionic fibers. Stimulation of these nerves causes a contraction of * " Skandinavischea Axchiv f. Physiologic, " 4, 241, 1892. KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 811 the small arteries of the kidney, a shrinkage in volume of the whole organ as measured by the oncometer (see Fig. 232), and a dimin- ished secretion of urine. When, on the other hand, these con- strictor fibers are cut as they enter the hilus of the kidney, the ar- teries are dilated on account of the removal of the tonic action of the constrictor fibers, the organ enlarges, and a greater quantity of blood passes through it, since the resistance to the blood-flow is diminished while the general arterial pressure in the aorta remains practically the same. Along with this greater flow of blood there is a marked increase in the secretion of urine. Under normal conditions we must suppose that these fibers are brought into play to a greater or less extent by reflex stimulation, and thus serve to control the blood-flow through the kidney and thereby influence its functional activity. It has been shown, too, that the kidney receives vasodilator nerve-fibers, — that is, fibers which when stimulated directly or reflexly cause a dilatation of the arteries, and therefore a greater flow of blood through the or- gan. According to Bradford, these fibers emerge from the spinal cord mainly in the anterior roots of the eleventh, twelfth, and thir- teenth thoracic spinal nerves. Under normal conditions these fibers are probably thrown into action by reflex stimulation and lead to an increased functional activity. It will be seen, therefore, that the kidneys possess a local nervous mechanism through which their secretory activity may be increased or diminished' by correspond- ing alterations in the blood-supply. So far as is known, this is the only way in which the secretion in the kidneys can be directly af- fected by the central nervous system. It should be borne in mind, also, that the blood-flow through the kidneys, and therefore their secretory activity, may be affected by conditions influencing general arterial pressure. Conditions such as asphyxia, strychnin poison- ing, or painful stimulation of sensory nerves, which cause a general vasoconstriction, influence the kidney in the same way, and tend, therefore, to diminish the flow of blood through it ; while conditions which lower general arterial pressure, such as general vascular dila- tation of the skin vessels, may also depress the secretory action of the kidney by diminishing the amount of blood flowing through it. In what way any given change in the vascular conditions of the body will influence the secretion of the kidney depends upon a num- ber of factors and their relations to one another, but any change which will increase the difference in pressure between the blood in the renal artery and the renal vein will tend to augment the flow of blood unless it is antagonized by a simultaneous constriction in the small arteries of the kidney itself. On the contrary, any vas- cular dilatation of the vessels in the kidney will tend to increase the blood-flow through it unless there is at the same time such a SI 2 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION* AND SECRETION*. general fall of blood-pressure as is sufficient to lower the pressure in the renal artery and reduce the driving force of the blood to an extent that more than counteracts the favorable influence of dimin- ished resistance in its small arteries. Although the kidney does not possess specific secretory nerve fibers, it is possible that there may be formed in the body specific chemical excitants or diuretics belonging to the general group of hormones (p. 749). Schafer* has recently shown that such a substance occurs normally in the nervous lobe of the pituitary gland, and it is possible that the internal secretion of this lobe may play toward kidney activity a role similar to that of the adrenalin toward muscular metabolism. The Composition of Urine. — The urine of man is a yellowish liquid that varies greatly in depth of color. It has an average specific gravity of 1.020 and usually an acid reaction. This acid reaction is attributed generally to the presence of acid phosphates, particularly acid sodium phosphate (NaH2P04); but, according to Folin,f the acidity is due partially and indeed in larger part to or- ganic acids. When tested by the usual indicators (litmus) human urine may show an alkaline reaction, and, in fact, observations indicate that the reaction may vary in accordance with the character of the food. Among carnivora the urine is uniformly acid, and among herbivora it is alkaline so long as they use a veg- etable diet. During starvation, however, or when living upon the mothers' milk,-r-that is, whenever they are existing upon a purely animal diet — the urine becomes acid. The general explanation of this effect of food that has been suggested (l)rechsel) is that upon an animal diet more acids are formed (from the oxidation of the sulphur and phosphorus of the proteids) than in the case of the vegetal )lc foods in which the alkaline salts of the vegetable acids give rise on oxidation in the body to alkaline carbonates. The kidney separates from the neutral blood and lymph the excess of acid salts and thus maintains a normal balance between the acid and basic equivalents in the blood, but the fact that on an ordinary mixed diet the urine has an acid reaction indicates that the acids formed in the body during metabolism must exceed the bases. The composition of the urine is very complex. In addition to the water and inorganic salts the following elements an; important, namely, urea, the purin bodies (uric acid, xanthin, hypoxantbin), creatinin, hippuric acid, oxalic acid (calcium oxalate), several conjugated sulphates and conjugated glycuronates, several aromatic OXyacidfl and nitrogenous acids, fatty acids, dissolved gases (N and C02)> :u"' the urinary pigments urochrome and urobilin. This list is not complete; a Dumber of additional substances have been de- * Schflfer and Herring, "Phil, Trans." L906, B. excix., 1. t "American Journal of Physiology," 9, -<).">, L903. KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 813 scribed as occurring constantly or occasionally in traces within the limits of health, and some substances are secreted whose composi- tion is unknown. Under pathological conditions the composition may be still further modified. The complexity of the composition may be understood when it is recalled that through this organ are eliminated some of all. the end-products formed in the various tis- sues, together with products arising from bacterial fermentation in the gastro-intestinal canal and various more or less foreign sub- stances taken with the food. It is not possible to describe all the numerous constituents that have been observed. Attention may be directed to those that quantitatively or otherwise are of chief physiological interest. The Nitrogen Elimination in the Urine. — Nearly all of the ex- cretion of nitrogen occurs in the urine. In the metabolism of the usual foodstuffs— carbohydrates, fats, and proteins — the end-prod- ucts of their destruction or physiological oxidation in the body are water, carbon dioxid, and nitrogenous waste products (and sulphates and phosphates from the sulphur and phosphorus in the proteins). The water is eliminated in the urine, the sweat, saliva, etc., and the expired air. The C02 is eliminated in the expired air, and in smaller part in dissolved form in the secretions (sweat, urine) . The nitrog- enous excretion, representing the breaking down of protein material, is found in minute part in the sweat, to a larger extent in the feces, but in by far the main amount in the urine. In all problems con- cerning protein metabolism in the body, both as regards its char- acter and extent, the quantitative study of this excretion is of par- amount importance. In order to determine the total amount of protein metabolism it is customary to determine the total nitrogen eliminated in the urine, without regard to its specific form. This determination is made usually by the method of Kjeldahl. The total weight of nitrogen multiplied by 6.25 gives the amount of pro- tein broken down, since nitrogen forms, on the average, 16 per cent, of the weight of the protein molecule. In an average-sized man the total nitrogen eliminated in a day varies, let us say, between 14 and 18 gms., which would correspond to 88 and 117 gms. of pro- tein. It being often necessary to distinguish between the forms in which this nitrogen is eliminated, the following distinctions are made: (1) The urea nitrogen, — that is, the nitrogen eliminated as urea. According to some recent analyses by Folin,* the urea nitrogen in man averages 87.5 per cent, of the total nitrogen. (2) The am- monia nitrogen, — that is, the nitrogen found in the form of am- monia salts which liberate free ammonia on the addition of a fixed alkali. The proportion of this ammonia nitrogen often varies, especially under pathological conditions affecting the liver. Its *" American Physiological Journal," 13, 45, 1905 S14 PHYSIOLOGY OF Dl'GESTION AND SECRETION. quantitative determination is a matter of importance. The aver- age amount in health may be stated (Folin) as 4.3 per cent, of the total nitrogen. (3) The creatinin nitrogen — that is, the amount excreted as creatinin and indicative of a special (muscular) metab- olism (3.6 per cent, of total nitrogen). (4) The purin nitrogen (uric acid, xanthin, hypoxanthin), also indicative of a special metabolism. (5) The unknown nitrogen. A considerable portion of the nitrogen is eliminated in compounds whose composition as yet has not been determined satisfactorily. According to some analyses this portion of the nitrogen may amount to more than 5 per cent, of the total nitrogen. The so-called oxyproteic acid constitutes a part of this unknown residue. This nitrogenous substance is said to yield leucin and other amino acids on hydroly- sis,* a fact which would suggest that it belongs to the protein group or is derived from a protein, possibly it is a polypeptid. Origin and Significance of Urea. — Urea has the formula, CO- N,H4. It may be considered as an amid of carbonic acid, and has, therefore, the structural formula of CCX^h2- It occurs in the urine in relatively large quantities (2 per cent.). As the total quan- tity of urine secreted in twenty-four hours by an adult male may be placed at from 1500 to 1700 c.c, it follows that from 30 to 34 gms. of urea are eliminated from the body during this period. It is the most important of the nitrogenous excreta of the body, the chief end-product, so far as the nitrogen is concerned, of the phys- iological metabolism of the proteins and the albuminoids of the foods and the tissues. If we know how much urea is secreted in a given period, we know approximately how much protein has been broken down in the body in the same time. In round numbers, 1 'Jin. of protein will yield l gm. of urea, as may be calculated easily from I lie amount of nitrogen contained in each. Since, however, some of the nitrogen of protein is eliminated in other forms uric acid, creatinin, etc even an exact determination of all the urea i- not sufficient to determine with accuracy the total amount of protein of all kind.- I hat has keen met abolized. This fact is arrived at more perfectly, as slated ahove, by a determination of the total nitrogen of 'he urine and ot her exc ret ions. In addition I o t he urine, urea ifi found in slighl quantities ill other secretions in milk (in trace I and in sweat. In the latter liquid the quantity of urea in twenty-four hours may be quite appreciable asmuch, for instance, as 0.8 gm. although such a large amount is found only after active exerci e. I' ha€ been ascertained definitely that urea is not formed by the kidneys; it is brought to the kidneys by the blood for elimi- nation. That urea is not made in the kidneys is demonstrated I on nil 'im berg, Hofmeiater's " Beitr&ge, " L0, 411, 1907. KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 815 by such facts as these: If blood, on the one hand, is irrigated through an isolated kidney, no urea is formed, even though substances (such as ammomium carbonate) from which urea is readily produced are added to the blood; on the other hand, urea is constantly present in the blood (0.0348 to 0.1529 per cent.), and if the two kidneys are removed, it continues to accumulate steadily in the blood as long as the animal survives. It has been ascertained that the urea is produced in part in the liver. The most important questions to be decided are: Through what steps is the protein molecule metabolized to the form of urea? and What is the antecedent substance brought to the liver, from which it makes urea? It is impossible to answer these questions perfectly, but recent investi- gations have thrown a great deal of light on the whole process, and they give hope that before long the entire history of the deriva- tion of urea from proteins will be known. The results of this work may be stated briefly as follows: 1 . Urea arises from ammonia salts which in the liver are converted to urea by a process equivalent to dehydration. It has long been known that when ammonium carbonate is added to blood perfused through a liver it is converted to urea.* The reaction may be represented as follows: ro^ONH, wo- m<^NH-2 LO N — C — N hydrogen compound of this nucleus would be designated as purin, * Folin, "American Journal of Physiology," 13, 117, 1905. 52 SIS PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. N = CH and would have the formula: HC C— XH , C5H4N4. Addi- X — C — N^CH tion of an atom of oxygen gives hypoxanthin, C5H4N40: HX — CO HC C — NH 11 '] -^yCH. Addition of two atoms of ox}Tgen gives xan- HX — CO thin, C5H4N40,: CO C — XH I II \ H^- q N ^CH. And addition of three atoms HX — CO of oxvgen gives uric acid, C5H.N403: CO C — XH , which I II >co HX — C — XH from this standpoint might be named trioxypurin. If one of the H atoms in the purin is substituted by an amino-groiip, NH2, the com- pound, adenin (C5H5N5), is obtained, and the substitution of an NH., group in hypoxanthin gives the compound guanin (C5H5N50). Moreover, caffein, the active principle of coffee and tea, and theo- bromin, the active principle of cocoa, are respectively trimethyl and dimethyl compounds of xanthin. We have to distinguish, therefore, three classes of purin compounds, namely, the oxypurins, comprising monoxypurin or hypoxanthin, dioxypurin or xanthin, and trioxypurin or uric acid; the aminopurins, comprising adenin or aminopurin and guanin or aminohypoxanthin, and the methyl- purins, comprising caffein or trimethyl xanthin (CHH10N4O., or C-H- (CH3)3N402) and theobromin or dimethyl xanthin (C7HsN,02 or C5H2(CH3)2N402). Uric acid, xanthin, and hypoxanthin are found constantly in the urine and in the feces small amounts of xanthin, hypoxanthin, adenin, and guanin may also occur. It has been pointed out* that these substances come partly from purin bodies taken as food, [f materials containing the purin bodies, such as meat, are fed, these bodies are excreted in pari in the urine. It is proposed to designate the uric acid, etc., thai has this origin as the exogenous purin material. A portion of the amount daily secreted comes, however, from a metabolism of the protein material of the body, and this portion may be distinguished as the endogenous purin bodies. This latter amounl is found to be practically constant', 0.15 to 0.20 gm. per day for any one individual, and the amount is not affected by change in the quantity or character of the food, but varies within certain limits with the manner of life. Evidently ♦ See J5uria.il and Schur, "Arehiv f. die geaammte Phyeiologie, " 94, 273, mo::. KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 819 the endogenous purin nitrogen represents a special metabolism, propably of the living tissues, that goes on independently, in great measure, of the mere oxidation of food. According to Siven, the production during sleep is much less than during the waking hours. Since the purin bodies may be obtained readily by hydrolytic cleavage of the nuclein or nucleic acid constituent of the nucleoproteins, and since nucleoprotein material or nucleins when fed to animals cause an increase in the amount of purin nitrogen eliminated in the urine, it is most probable that in the body these purin bases represent the end-products of the metabolism of nuclein material. The intermediate processes in this metabolism, whether it affects the nuclein taken as food or the nuclein contained within the tissues of the body, are supposed to take place according to the following general schema: The nucleins that are split off from the nucleoprotein are acted upon first by an enzyme, nuclease, which has been demonstrated to exist in various tissues, e. g., in the spleen, liver, lungs, and kidneys. By the action of this enzyme the nuclein is split with the formation of some of the purin bodies, adenin, guanin, xanthin, or hypoxan- thin. The adenin and guanin are then deamidized and converted respectively to hypoxanthin and xanthin. Jones * has given reasons to believe that two specific deamidizing enzymes of this character may exist in the body, namely, adenase and guanase. Their action may be represented by the following equations : C5H5N5 + H20 = CsH4N40 + NH3 Adenin. Hypoxanthin. C5H5N50 + H20 = C5H4N402 + NH3 Guanin. Xanthin. The hypoxanthin and xanthin thus formed are in turn oxidized to uric acid by the action of an oxidase to which the specific name of xanthinoxidase has been given. Its action upon the hypoxanthin or xanthin is represented by the series: C5H4N40 + O = C5H(N402 Hypoxanthin. Xanthin. C3H4N"402 + O = C5H4N403 Xanthin. Uric acid. Finally, as stated above, it can be shown that a portion of the uric acid may be further metabolized by the action of a specific urico- lytic enzyme and give rise to urea. The portion of the uric acid under- going this last change varies in different animals, as may be demon- strated by giving definite amounts of uric acid in the food. Ex- periments of this kind have shown that in man about one-half of the uric acid formed gives rise to urea, while in dogs and cats only about -^ suffers this change. In rabbits the proportion is £. According to a former view (Horbaczewsky) it was supposed that * Jones and Austrian, "Zeitschrift f. physio! . Chem.," 1906, xlviii., 110. SJH PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. the endogenous purin nitrogen represents an end-product of the metabolism of the nuclein found in the nuclei of cells, especially in the nuclei of the leucocytes. But Burian has shown, on the contrary, that most of this nitrogen in the excreta arises from a metabolism in the muscular tissues* Increased muscular activity is followed within an hour or two by an increased output of uric acid, and when an isolated muscle is perfused with a mixture of defibrinated blood and Ringer's sedation, uric acid is given off to the circulating liquid. When the muscle under these last-mentioned conditions is made to work a distinct increase in the hypoxanthin and uric acid can be determined. It would seem, therefore, that under normal conditions the uric acid and other purin bases are derived mainly from a metabolism of the muscular substance whereby hypoxanthin is produced. This substance is then oxidized to uric acid and a part of the uric acid is further changed to urea.t Origin and Significance of the Creatinin and Creatin. — Creatinin (C4H7N30) occurs in the urine, and it has been assumed that it is derived from the creatin (C4H9N30,) found in muscle. Its /NH — CO structural formula is given as NHC/ and its chemical re- XN(CH3)CH2 lations are indicated by the fact that it may be prepared synthetically from methyl-glycocoll and cyanamid, — that is, the union of these two substances gives creatin, from which in turn creatinin may be obtained. X=C-NH2 + NH(CH8)CH2COOH - NHC<3^£H/X)0Hi Cyanamid. Methyl-glycocoll. Creatin. Creatinin occurs in the urine constantly and in amounts equal to 1 in 2 gms. per day, or, according to Shaffer, J there is an excretion of from 7 to 11 nig. of creatinin nitrogen per kilogram of body- weight. Next to the urea and the ammonia compounds it forms the mosl important of the known nitrogenous constituent of the urine. Its physiological history is imperfectly known. Under constanl conditions of life the amounl of creatinin formed in the body i independenl of the quantity of protein eaten, and this fact indicates (Folin) thai it represents an end-product of the metabolism of living or organized protein tissue rather than one of the results of the metabolism of the food protein. This con- elusion is strengthened by the fad that in fevers and other patho- * Burian, "Zeitachrift f. physiol. Chemie," \liii., p. ■r>'-'<~- (•For :i review of the exten ive literature, see Block, "Bioehemisches Centralblatt." 1906, v., Nob. 12 I I Con nil Shaffer, "American Journal of Physiology," '■>'■'>, I, L908. KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 821 logical conditions in which there is an increased breaking down of tissues the creatinin excretion is increased.* As stated above, the usual view has been that the creatinin of the urine is derived from the creatin of the muscles, but the effort to demonstrate that this relationship actually exists has met with many difficulties. The older observers pointed out what seems to be an objection to this view, namely, the lack of relationship between the amount of creatin in the musculature of the body (about 90 gms.) and the small amount of creatinin (1 to 2 gms.) excreted daily. If the creatin is a nitrogenous waste constantly formed at this rate and excreted as creatinin, there ought to be a larger amount of the latter substance. To meet this difficulty it was suggested that some of the creatin may be converted to urea, but as a matter of fact the possibility of such a conversion in the body has not been demonstrated. Moreover, the conversion by the body of creatin into creatinin is not so simple a matter as was supposed. When creatinin is added to the diet it is excreted as creatinin. When creatin, on the contrary, is fed, there is no apparent increase in the creatinin of the urine. In fact, in the experiments reported the creatin nitrogen was not recovered in any form in the urine. The newer analyses seem also to show that normal muscular work causes no increase in the excretion of creatinin in the urine. Experimental work, in fact, upon the relationship, if any, and significance of the creatin and creatinin has got only so far as to show that the story is more complex and difficult than was formerly supposed. Normally, creatin exists in the muscular tissue of the vertebrate animals — not in that of the invertebrates. Creatinin, on the contrary, does not occur in detectable amounts in the blood or tissues of the body, but is a constant constituent of the urine. Creatin is not present normally in the urine, but under conditions which involve a destruction of the organized body-proteins, for example, in fevers, starvation, in women after delivery, during the period of involution of the uterus, etc., it may be secreted by the kidney in distinct amounts. Several investigators have assumed that the liver is concerned in the history of these two substances, but the part played by this organ is interpreted differently by those working at the subject; and it seems abso- lutely necessary at present to suspend judgment in regard to the connection and significance of these two substances until inves- tigations have reached more satisfactory results, f Hippuric Acid. — This substance has the formula C9H9N03. Its molecular structure is known, since upon decomposition it yields * Hoogenhuyze and Verploegh, "Zeitschrift f. physiol. Chemie, " 57, 161, 1908. f For a recent review and the literature, consult Mendel, "Science," April 9, 1909. 822 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. benzoic acid and glycocoli, and, moreover, it may be produced syn- thetically by the union of these two substances. Hippuric acid may be described, therefore, as a benzoyl-amino-acetic acid (CH2- NH[CGH5CO]COOH). It is found in considerable quantities in the urine of herbivorous animals (1.5 to 2.5 per cent.), and in much smaller amounts in the urine of man and of the carnivora. In human mine, on an average diet, about 0.7 gm. are excreted in twenty-four hours. If the diet is Largely vegetable, this amount may be much increased. This last fact is readily explained, for it has been found that if benzoic acid or substances containing this grouping are fed to animals they appear in the urine as hippuric acid. Evi- dently a synthesis occurs in the body, and Bunge and Schmie- deberg proved conclusively that in dogs the union of benzoic acid and glycocoli to form hippuric acid takes place in the kidney itself. Later it was discovered* that the same synthesis may be effected by ground-up kidney tissue, mixed with blood and kept under oxygen pressure. It seems possible, therefore, that the synthesis is due to some specific constituent of the kidney cells, possibly an enzyme. Vegetable foods contain benzoic acid com- pounds, and we can understand, therefore, why when fed they in- crease the hippuric acid output of the urine. Since, however, in starving animals or animals fed upon meat hippuric acid is still present in the urine, although reduced in amount, it is evident that it arises in part as a result of the body metabolism. It should be added finally that some of the hippuric acid may be derived from the process of protein putrefaction that occurs in the large intestine. The Conjugated Sulphates and the Sulphur Excretion. — The sulphur excretion of the urine possesses an importance similar to that of nitrogen. Sulphur constitutes an element in most of the proteins, and in some form, therefore, it will be represented in the end-products of protein metabolism. The sulphur elimination in the urine, like I be nil rogen eliminal ion, has been taken as a measure of the amount of protein destruction. In the urine the sulphur occur- in three forms: (1) In an oxidized form as inorganic sul- phates. Some of the sulphates are undoubtedly derived or may be derived from the mineral sulphates ingested with the food, bu1 the larger pari arises from the oxidation of the sulphur of the proteins. (2 The 30 called conjugated or ethereal sulphates are combinations een lulphuric acid and Indoxyl, skatoxyl, phenol, and cresol, giving us phenoisulphuric acid (C6H8OS02OH), cresolsulphuric acid "II OS02OH), Indoxylsulphuric acid or indican (C8HaNOS02OH), and skatoxylsulphuric acid (C9H8NOS02OH). The indol, skatol, phenol, and cresol are formed in i he large intestine as a result of bac- terial putrefaction. They are eliminated in part in the feces, but * Ba hford and Cramer. "Zeit ichrift f. phyi iol. Chemie," 35, 324, 1902. KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 823 in part are absorbed into the blood, and after oxidation are conjugated with sulphuric acid and eliminated in the urine. The process of conjugation is valuable from a physiological standpoint, as it converts substances having an injurious action into harmless compounds. It should be added, also, that to a small extent the phenol, incloxyl, and skatoxyl may be secreted in the urine as con- jugated glucuronates, — that is, in combination with glycuronic acid (C6H10O7), a reducing substance closely connected with dextrose. From a nutritional standpoint the amount of these substances pres- ent furnishes a measure of the extent of protein putrefaction in the intestine, by virtue of the indol and phenol constituents. All con- ditions that increase the putrefactive processes in the intestine are accompanied by a parallel increase in the ethereal sulphates. By virtue of the sulphuric acid component these bodies represent also one of the forms in which sulpftur is excreted from the body. (3) Some of the sulphur in the urine may occur in unoxid- ized form as sulphocyanid or as ethyl-sulphide (Abel) ([C2H5]2S). Under certain pathological conditions (cystinuria) some sulphur may be excreted in the form of cystin, but this is not a normal con- stituent of the urine. For other most interesting and significant changes in the composition of the urine under pathological condi- tions reference must be made to special works upon the urine or upon pathological chemistry. Water and Inorganic Salts. — Water is lost from the body through three main channels, — namely, the lungs, the skin, and the kidney, the last of these being the most important. The quan- tity of water lost through the lungs probably varies within small limits only. The quantity lost through the sweat varies, of course, with the temperature, with exercise, etc., and it may be said that the amounts of water secreted through kidney and skin stand in something of an inverse proportion to each other ; that is, the greater the quantity lost through the skin, the less will be secreted by the kidneys. Through these three organs, but mainly through the kidneys, the blood is being continually depleted of water, and the loss must be made up by the ingestion of new water. When water is swallowed in excess the superfluous amount is rapidly eliminated through the kidneys. The amount of water secreted may be in- creased by the action of diuretics, such as potassium nitrate and caffein. The inorganic salts of urine consist chiefly of the chlorids, phos- phates, and sulphates of the alkalies and the alkaline earths. It may be said, in general, that they arise partly from the salts ingested with the food, and are eliminated from the blood by the kidney in the water secretion; and in part they are formed in the destruc- tive metabolism that takes place in the body, particularly that $24 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. involving the proteins and related bodies. Sodium chlorid occurs in the largest quantities, averaging about 15 gms. per day, of which the larger part, doubtless, is derived directly from the salt taken in the food. The phosphates occur in combination with cal- cium and magnesium, but chiefly as the acid phosphates of sodium or potassium. The acid reaction of the urine is usually attributed to these latter substances. The phosphates result in part from the destruction of phosphorus-containing tissues in the body, but chiefly from the phosphates of the food. The sulphates of urine are found partly in an oxidized form as simple sulphates or con- jugated with organic compounds, as described above. Micturition. — The urine is secreted continuously by the kid- neys, is carried to the bladder through the ureters, and is then at intervals finally ejected from the bladder through the urethra by the act of micturition. Movements of the Ureters. — The ureters possess a muscular coat consisting of an internal longitudinal and external circular layer. The contractions of this muscular coat form the means by which the urine is driven from the pelvis of the kidney into the bladder. The movements of the ureter have been carefully studied by Engel- mann.* According to his description, the musculature of the ureter contracts spontaneously at intervals of ten to twent}'" seconds (rab- bit), the contraction beginning at the kidney and progressing toward the bladder in the form of a peristaltic wave and with a velocity of about 20 to 30 mms. per second. The result of this movement should be the forcing of the urine into the bladder in a series of gentle, rhythmical spurts, and this method of filling the bladder has been observed in the human being. Suter and Mayerf report some observations upon a boy in whom there was ectopia of the bladder, with exposure of the orifices of the ureters. The flow into the bladder was intermittent and was about equal upon the two sides for the time the child was under observation (three and a half days). The causation of the contractions of the ureter musculature is not easily explained. Engelmann finds that artificial stimulation of th<- ureter or of ii piece of the ureter may start peristaltic con- tractions which move in both directions from the point stimulated. He was not able to find ganglion cells in the upper two-thirds of the ureter and was led to believe, therefore, that the contraction orig- inates in the muscular tissue independently of. extrinsic or intrinsic nerve-, and thai the contraction wave propagates itself directly from muscle cell to muscle cell, the entire musculature behaving *"PflUger' Arcbiv f . die gesammte Phy iologie, " 2, 243, L 869, and 4, 33; see al o Luca , ' Mnerican Journal of Physiology," 17, 392, L906. "Archivf. e per Pathologic und Pharmakologie," 32, 241, L893. KIDNEY AND ,SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 825 as though it were a single, colossal, hollow muscle fiber. The liber- ation of the stimulus which inaugurates the normal peristalsis of the ureter seems to be connected with the accumulation of urine in its upper or kidney portion. It may be supposed that the urine that collects at this point as it flows from the kidney stimulates the muscular tissue to contraction, either by its pressure or in some other way, and thus leads to an orderly sequence of contraction waves. It is possible, however, that the muscle of the ureter, like that of the heart, is spontaneously contractile under normal conditions, and does not depend upon the stimulation of the urine. Thus, according to Engelmann, section of the ureter near the kidney does not materially affect the nature of the contractions of the stump attached to the kidney, although in this case the pressure of the urine could scarcely act as a stimulus. Moreover, in the case of the rat, in which the ureter is highly contractile, the tube may be cut into several pieces and each piece will continue to exhibit period- ical peristaltic contractions. It does not seem possible at present to decide between these two views as to the cause of the contrac- tions. The nature of the contractions, their mode of progression, and the way in which they force the urine through the ureter seem, however, to be clearly established. Efforts to show a regulatory action upon these movements through the central nervous system have so far given negative results. Movements of the Bladder. — The bladder contains a muscular coat of plain muscle tissue, which, according to the usual description, is arranged so as to make an external longitudinal coat and an internal circular or oblique coat. A thin, longitudinal layer of muscle tissue lying to the interior of the circular coat is also de- scribed. The separation between the longitudinal and circular layers is not so definite as in the case of the intestine; they seem, in fact, to form a continuous layer, one passing gradually into the other by a change in the direction of the fibers. At the cervix the circular layer is strengthened, and has been supposed to act as a sphincter with regard to the urethral orifice — the so-called sphinc- ter vesicae internus. Around the urethra just outside the blad- der is a circular layer of striated muscle that is frequently desig- nated as the external sphincter or sphincter urethras. The urine brought into the bladder accumulates within its cavity to a certain limit. It is prevented from escaping through the urethra at first by the mere elasticity of the parts at the urethral orifice, aided per- haps by tonic .contraction of the internal sphincter, although this function of the circular layer is disputed by some observers. When the accumulation becomes greater the external sphincter is brought into action. If the desire to urinate is strong the external sphincter seems undoubtedly to be controlled by voluntary effort, but whether 826 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AX*) SECRETION". or not, in moderate filling of the bladder, it is brought into play by an involuntary reflex is not definitely determined. Backflow of urine from the bladder into the ureters is effectually prevented by the oblique course of the ureters through the Avail of the blad- der. Owing to this circumstance, pressure within the bladder serves to close the mouths of the ureters, and, indeed, the more completely, the higher the pressure. At some point in the filling of the bladder the pressure is sufficient to arouse a conscious sen- sation of fullness and a desire to micturate. Under normal condi- tions the act of micturition follows. It consists essentially in a strong contraction of the bladder, with a simultaneous relaxation of the external sphincter, if this muscle is in action, the effect of which is to obliterate more or less completely the cavity of the blad- der and drive the urine out through the urethra. The force of this contraction is considerable, as is evidenced by the height to which the urine may spurt from the end of the urethra. According to Mosso, the contraction may support, in the dog, a column of liquid two meters high. The contractions of the blad- der may be and usually are assisted by contractions of the walls of the abdomen, especially toward the end of the act. As in defeca- tion and vomiting, the contraction of the abdominal muscles, when the glottis is closed so as to keep the diaphragm fixed, serves to in- crease the pressure in the abdominal and pelvic cavities, and thus assists in or completes the emptying of the bladder. It is, however, not an essential part of the act of micturition. The last portions of the urine escaping into the urethra are ejected, in the male, in spurts produced by the rhythmical contractions of the bulbocavernosus muscle. Considerable uncertainty and difference of opinion exists as to the physiological mechanism by which this series of muscular con- tractions, and especially the contractions of the bladder itself, are produced. According to the frequently quoted description given by Golfcz,* the scries of events is as follows: The distention of the bladder by the urine causes finally a stimulation of the sensory fibers of the organ and produces a reflex contraction of the blad- der musculature which squeezes some urine into the urethra. The fii-i drops, however, that enter the urethra stimulate the sensory nerves there and give rise to a conscious desire to urinate. If no cle i- presented the bladder then empties itself, assisted per- haps by the contractions of the abdominal muscles. The emptying of the bladder may, however, be prevented, if desirable, by a Volun- tary contraction of the sphincter urethras, which opposes the effect of the contraction of the bladder. If the bladder is not too full and the sphincter is kepi in :ic.tion for some time, the contractions 'Archiv f. die gesammte Phj iologie," 8, 478, 1874. KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 827 of the bladder may cease and the desire to micturate pass off. Ac- cording to this view, the voluntary control of the process is limited to the action of the external sphincter and the abdominal muscles; the contraction of the bladder itself is purely an unconscious reflex taking place through a lumbar center. The experiments of Goltz and others, upon dogs in which the spinal cord was severed at the junction of the lumbar and the tho- racic regions, indicate that micturition is essentially a reflex act, with its center in the lumbar cord, although the same observer has shown that in dogs whose spinal cord has been entirely destroyed, except in the cervical and upper thoracic region, the bladder emp- ties itself normally without the aid of external stimulation. Mosso and Pellacani* have made experiments upon women in which a catheter was introduced into the bladder and connected with a record- ing apparatus to measure the volume of the bladder. Their ex- periments indicate that the sensation of fullness and desire to micturate come from sensory stimulation, in the bladder itself, caused by the pressure of the urine. They point out that the bladder is very sensitive to reflex stimulation; that even* psychical act and every sensor}* stimulus is apt to cause a contraction or in- creased tone of the bladder. The bladder is therefore subject to continual changes in size from reflex stimulation, and the pressure within it will depend not simply on the quantity of urine, but on the condition of tone of its muscles. At a certain pressure the sensory nerves are stimulated and under normal conditions mictu- rition ensues. We may understand, from this point of view, how it happens that we have sometimes a strong desire to micturate when the bladder contains but little urine, — for example, under emotional excitement. In such cases if the micturition is prevented, probably by the action of the external sphincter, the bladder may sub- sequently relax and the sensation of fullness and desire to micturate pass away until the urine accumulates in sufficient quantity, or the pressure is again raised by some circumstance which causes a reflex contraction of the bladder. Nervous Mechanism. — According to Langley and Anderson.t the bladder in cats, dogs, and rabbits receives motor fibers from two sources: (1) From the lumbar nerves, the fibers passing out in the second to the fifth lumbar nerves and reaching the bladder through the sympathetic chain and the inferior mesenteric ganglion and the hypogastric nerves and plexus (Fig. 277). Stimulation of these nerves causes a comparatively feeble contraction of the blad- der. (2) From the sacral spinal nerves, the fibers originating in the second and third sacral spmal nerves, or in the rabbit in * "Archives italiennes de biologie, " 1, 1S82. t " Journal of Physiology, " 19, 71, 1895. 828 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. the third and fourth, and taking their course through the so-called nervi erigentes and the hypogastric plexus. Stimulation of these nerves, or some of them, causes strong contractions of the blad- der, sufficient to empty its contents. Little evidence was obtained of the presence of vasomotor fibers. According to Nawrocki and Skabitschewsky,* the spinal sensory fibers to the bladder are found in part in the posterior roots of the first, second, third, and fourth sacral spinal nerves, particularly the second and third. When these fibers are stimulated they excite reflexly the motor fibers to the bladder found in the anterior roots of the second and third sacral spinal nerves. Some sensory fibers to the bladder may pass by way of the hypogastric nerves. When the central stump of one hypogastric nerve is stimulated it produces, according to these authors, a reflex effect upon the motor fibers in the other hypo- gastric nerve, causing a contraction of the bladder, the reflex oc- curring through the inferior mesenteric ganglion. This observa- tion has been confirmed by several authorities, but has been ex- plained by Langley and Anderson as a pseudoreflex or axon reflex (see p. 152). According to Elliott the innervation of the bladder varies in the different mammals. Speaking generally, the fibers passing by way of the nervi erigentes when stimulated cause contraction of the bladder (and inhibition of the sphincter). These fibers, therefore, are mainly concerned in the act of micturi- tion. The fibers supplied through the hypogastric nerve, on the contrary, cause mainly relaxation of the bladder musculature, and their stimulation, by inhibiting the tonus of the musculature, would seem to provide a means for holding the urine. The immediate spinal center through which the contractions of the bladder max' lie reflexly stimulated or inhibited lies, accord- ing to the experiments of ( roltz, in the lumbar portion of the cord, probably between the second and fifth lumbar spinal nerves. In dogs in which this portion of the cord was isolated by a cross- section at the junction of the thoracic and lumbar regions, mic- turition still ensued when the bladder was sufficiently full, and it could be called forth reflexly by sensory stimuli, especially by Blight irritation of the anal region. This localization lias been confirmed by others X but Elliot stales thai the sacral portion of the cord, which gives rise to the fibers of the nervi erigentes, may also serve as a reflex center for the bladder. Excretory Functions of the Skin. — The physiological activi- ties of the .-kin are varied. It forms, in the first place, a sensory surface covering the body, and interposed, as it were, between the *"Archivf. die gesammte Physiologie, " 49, 111, L891. i Elliot, "Journal of Physiology," ■ >■'>, 367, 1907. Stewart, "American Journal of Physiology, " 2, L82, 1899. KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 829 external world and the inner mechanism. Nerve fibers of pressure, temperature, and pain are distributed over its surface, and bv means of these fibers reflexes of various kinds are effected which keep the body adapted to changes in its environment. The physiology of the skin from this standpoint is discussed in the section on special senses. Again, the skin plays a part of immense value to the body in regulating the body temperature. This regulation, which is effected by variations in the blood supply or the sweat secretion, is described at appropriate places in the sections on Nutrition and Circulation. In the female, during the period of lactation, the mam- mary glands, which must be reckoned among the organs of the skin, form an important secretion, the milk; the physiology of this gland is referred to in the section on Reproduction. In this section we are concerned with the physiology of the skin from a different standpoint, — namely, as an excretory organ. The excretions of the skin are formed in the sweat-glands and the sebaceous glands. Sweat. — The sweat or perspiration is a secretion of the sweat glands. These latter structures are found over the entire cutaneous surface except in the deeper portions of the external auditory meatus, the prepuce, and the glans penis. They are particularly abundant upon the palms of the hands and the soles of the feet. Krause estimates that their total number for the whole cutaneous surface is about two millions. In man they are formed on the type of simple tubular glands; the terminal portion contains the secretory cells, and at this part the tube is usually coiled to make a more or less compact knot, thus increasing the extent of the secreting sur- face. The larger ducts have a thin, muscular coat of involuntary tissue that may possibly be concerned in the ejection of the secre- tion. The secretory cells in the terminal portion are columnar in shape, possess a granular cytoplasm, and are arranged in a single layer. The amount of secretion formed by these glands varies greatly, being influenced by the condition of the atmosphere as re- gards temperature and moisture, as well as by various physical and psychical states, such as exercise and emotions. The average quan- tity for twenty-four hours is said to vary between 700 and 900 gms., although this amount may be doubled under certain conditions. According to an interesting paper by Schierbeck,* the average quantity of sweat in twenty-four hours may amount to 2 to 3 liters in a person clothed, and therefore with an average temperature of 32° C. surrounding the skin. This author states that the amount of sweat given off from the skin in the form of insensible perspira- tion increases proportionately with the temperature until a certain critical point is reached (about 33° C. in the person investigated) , * "Archiv f. Physiologie," 1893, 116; see also Willebrand, " Skandi- navisches Archiv f. Physiologie," 13, 337, 1902. 830 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. when there is a marked increase in the water eliminated, the in- crease being simultaneous with the formation of visible sweat. At the same tune there is a sudden increase in the CO, eliminated from the skin. It is possible that the sudden increase in CO, is an in- dication of greater metabolism in the sweat glands in connection with the formation of visible sweat. Composition of the Secretion. — The precise chemical composition of sweat is difficult to determine, owing to the fact that as usually obtained it is liable to be mixed with the sebaceous secretion. Nor- mally it is a very thin secretion of low specific gravity (1.004) and an alkaline reaction, although when first secreted the reaction may be acid owing to admixture with the sebaceous material. The larger part of the inorganic salts consists of sodium chloric!. Small quantities of the alkaline sulphates and phosphates are also pres- ent. The organic constituents, though present in mere traces, are quite varied in number. Urea, uric acid, creatinin, aromatic oxy- acids, ethereal sulphates of phenol and skatol, and albumin, are said to occur when the sweating is profuse. Argutinsky has shown that after the action of vapor baths, and as the result of muscular work, the amount of urea eliminated in this secretion may be con- siderable. Under pathological conditions involving a diminished elimination of urea through the kidneys it has been observed that the amount found in the sweat is markedly increased, so that crys- tals of it may be deposited upon the skin. Under perfectly nor- mal conditions, however, it is obvious that the organic constituents are of minor importance. The main fact to be considered in the secretion of sweat is the formation of water. Secretory Fibers to the Sweat Glands. — Definite experimental proof of the existence of sweat nerves was first obtained by Goltz* in some experiments upon stimulation of the sciatic nerve in cats. In the cat and dog. in which sweat glands occur on the balls of the feet, the presence of sweat nerves may be demonstrated with great ease. Electrical stimulation of the peripheral end of the divided sciatic nerve, if sufficiently strong, will cause visible drops of sweat to form on the hairless skin of the balls of the feet. When the flirt codes are kept at the same spot on the nerve and the stimula- tion is maintained the secretion soon ceases; but this effect seems to be due to a temporary injury of some kind to the nerve fillers at the point of stimulation, and not to a genuine fatigue of the sweat glands or the sweat filters, since moving the electrodes to a new point on the nerve farther toward the periphery calls forth a new scent ion. The secret ion so formed is thin and limpid, and has a marked alkaline read ion. The anatomical course of I bese libers * "ArHiiv I', die gesammte Physiologic," ll, 71, L875. KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 831 has been worked out in the cat with great care by Langley.* He finds that for the hind feet they leave the spinal cord chiefly in the first and second lumbar nerves, enter the sympathetic chain, and emerge from this as postganglionic fibers in the gray rami which pass from the sixth lumbar to the second sacral ganglion, but chiefly in the seventh lumbar and first sacral, and then join the nerves of the sciatic plexus. For the forefeet the fibers leave the spinal cord in the fourth to the tenth thoracic nerves, enter the sympathetic chain, pass upward to the first thoracic ganglion, whence they are continued as postganglionic fibers that pass out of this ganglion by the gray rami communicating with the nerves forming the brachial plexus. The action of the nerve fibers upon the sweat glands can not be explained as an indirect effect, — for instance, as a result of a variation in the blood-flow. Experiments have repeatedly shown that, in the cat, stimulation of the sciatic still calls forth a secre- tion after the blood has been shut off from the leg by ligation of the aorta, or indeed after the leg has been amputated for as long as twenty minutes. So in human beings it is known that profuse sweating may often accompany a pallid skin, as in terror or nausea, while, on the other hand, the flushed skin of fever is char- acterized by the absence of perspiration. There seems to be no doubt that the sweat nerves are genuine secretory fibers, causing a secretion in consequence of a direct action on the cells of the sweat glands. In accordance with this physiological fact histological work has demonstrated that, special nerve fibers are supplied to the glandular epithelium. According to Arnstein, the terminal fibers form a small, branching, varicose ending in contact with the epithelial cells. The sweat gland may be made 'to secrete in many ways other than by direct artificial excitation of the sweat fibers, — for example, by external heat, dyspnea, muscular exercise, strong emotions, and by the action of various drugs, such as pilocarpin, muscarin, strychnin, nicotin, picrotoxin, and physostigmin. In all such cases the effect is supposed to result from an action on the sweat fibers, either directly on their terminations or indirectly upon their cells of origin in the central nervous system. In ordinar}' life the usual cause of profuse sweating is a high external temper- ature or muscular exercise. With regard to the former it is known that the high temperature does not excite the sweat glands im- mediately, but through the intervention of the central nervous system. If the nerves going to a limb be cut, exposure of that limb to a high temperature does not cause a secretion, showing that the temperature change alone is not sufficient to excite the gland or its terminal nerve fibers. We must. suppose, therefore, that the high temperature acts upon the sensory cutaneous nerves, * "Journal of Physiology," 12, 347, 1891. S32 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. possibly the heat fibers, and reflexly stimulates the sweat fibers. Although external temperature does not directly excite the glands, it should be stated that it affects their irritability either by direct action on the gland cells or upon the terminal nerve fibers. At a sufficiently low temperature the cat's paw does not secrete at all, and the Irritability of the glands is increased by a rise of temper- ature up to about 45° C. Dyspnea, muscular exercise, emotions, and many drugs affect the secretion, probably by action on the nerve centers. Pilocarpin, on the contrary, is known to stimulate the endings of the nerve fibers in the glands, while atropin has the opposite effect, com- pletely paralyzing the secretory fibers. Sweat Centers in the Central Nervous System. — The fact that secretion of sweat may be occasioned by stimulation of afferent nerves or by direct action upon the central nervous system, as in the case of dyspnea, implies the existence of physiological centers controlling the secretory fibers. The precise location of the sweat center or centers has not, however, been satisfactorily determined. Histologically and anatomically the arrangement of the sweat fibers resembles that of the vasoconstrictor fibers, and, reasoning from analogy, one might suppose the existence of a general sweat center in the medulla comparable to the vasoconstrictor center, but positive evidence of the existence of such an arrangement is lacking. It has been shown that when the medulla is separated from the cord by a section in the cervical or thoracic region the action of dyspnea, or of various sudorific drugs supposed to act on the central nervous system, may still cause a secretion. On the evidence of results of this character it is assumed that there are spinal sweat centers; but whether these are few in number or represent simply the various nuclei of origin of the fibers to different regions is not definitely known. It is possible that in addition to these spinal centers there is a general regulating center in the medulla. Sebaceous Secretion. — The sebaceous glands are simple or compound alveolar glands found over the cutaneous surface, usually in association with the hairs, although in some cases they occur separately, as, for instance, on the prepuce and glans penis, and on the lips. When they occur with the hairs the short duct opens into the hair follicle, so that the secretion is passed out upon the hair near the point at which it projects from the skin. The alveoli are filled with cuboidal or polygonal epithelial cells, which are arranged iii everal layers. Those uearesl (lie lumen of the gland are filled with fatty material. These cells are supposed to be cast off bodily, their detritus going to form the secretion. New cells are formed from the layer nearest the basement membrane, and thus the glands continue to produce a slow but continuous secretion. The sebaceous KIDNEY AND SKIN AS EXCRETORY ORGANS. 833 secretion, or sebum, is an oily, semiliquid material that sets, upon exposure to the air, to a cheesy mass, as is seen in the comedones or pimples which so frequently occur upon the skin from occlusion of the opening of the ducts. The exact composition of the secretion is not known. It contains fats and soaps, some cholesterin, albu- minous material (part of which is a nucleo-albumin often described as a casein), remnants of epithelial cells, and inorganic salts. The cholesterin occurs in combination with a fatty acid, and is found in especially large quantities in sheep's wool, from which it is extracted and used commercially under the name of lanolin. The sebaceous secretion from different places, or in different animals, is probably somewhat variable in composition as well as in quantity. The secretion of the prepuce is known as the smegma prceputii; that of the external auditory meatus, mixed with the secretion of the neigh- boring sweat glands or ceruminous glands, forms the well-known earwax or cerumen. The secretion in this place contains a reddish pigment of a bitterish-sweet taste, the composition of which has not been investigated. Upon the skin of the newly born the se- baceous material is accumulated to form the vernix caseosa. The well-known uropygal gland of birds is homologous with the mam- malian sebaceous glands, and its secretion has been obtained in sufficient quantities for chemical analysis. Physiologically it is believed that the sebaceous secretion affords a protection to the skin and hairs. Its oily character doubtless serves to protect the hairs from becoming too brittle, or, on the other hand, from being too easily saturated with external moisture. In this way it prob- ably aids in making the hairy coat a more perfect protection against the effect of external changes of temperature. Upon the surface of the skin, also, it forms a thin, protective layer that tends to prevent undue loss of heat from evaporation of the sweat and possibly is important in other ways in maintaining the physiological integrity of the external surface. Excretion of C02. — In some of the lower animals — the frog, for example — the skin takes an important part in the respiratory exchanges, eliminating C02 and absorbing 0. In man, and pre- sumably in the mammalia generally, it has been ascertained that changes of this kind are very slight. Estimates of the amount of C02 given off from the skin of man during twenty-four hours vary greatly, but the amount is small, about 7 to 8 gms. in twenty-four hours, unless there is marked sweating, in which case the amount is noticeably increased. 53 CHAPTER XLVI. SECRETION OF THE DUCTLESS GLANDS-INTERNAL SECRETION. The term "internal secretion" is used to designate those secre- tions of glandular tissues which, instead of being carried off to the exterior by a duct, are eliminated in the blood or lymph. The idea that secretory products may be given off in this way has long been held in reference to the ductless glands, such as the thyroid, pitui- tary body, etc., the absence of a duct suggesting naturally such a possibility. The term, however, seems to have been employed first by Claude Bernard, who emphasized the distinction between the ordinary secretions, or external secretions, and this group of internal secretions. Modern interest in the latter is due largely to work done by Brown-Sequard (1889) upon testicular extracts, work which itself was of doubtful value. This author was led to amplify the conception of an internal secretion by the assumption that all tissues give off a something to the blood which is characteristic, and is of importance in general nutrition. This idea led in turn to a revival of some old notions regarding the treatment of diseases of the different organs by extracts of the corresponding tissue, a therapeutical method usually designated as opotherapy. Brown- S6quard's extension of the idea of internal secretion has not been justified by subsequent work, and to-day we must limit the term to tissues that have a glandular structure. Experience has shown, however, that not only the duel less glands, but some at least of the typical glands provided with duds may give rise to internal secre- tions, 1 he pancreas, for example. In some of the ductless glands, on the '-out rary, the existence or non-existence of an internal secre- tion is -till :m open question. The work done since 1889 has, how- ever, demonstrated fully that some of the ductless glands play a file of the very greatest importance in general nutrition, and this knowledge has proved useful in widening our conception of the nutritional relatione in the organism and besides has found a valuable application in practical medicine. The conception that certain glandular organs may give rise to chemical products which on entering the circulation influence the activity of one or more other organs has recently found a fruitful application in the study of the digestive secretions. The gastric and pancreatic secretins may 834 SECRETION OF THE DUCTLESS GLANDS. 835 be regarded as examples of internal secretions. Chemical products of this kind which stimulate the activity of special organs Starling designates as hormones* From this point of view the active substances formed in the thyroids, adrenal glands, etc., may all be classified as specific hormones. Starling suggests that this means of coordinating the activities of the various parts of a complex organism may be regarded as the most primitive, while the better-known coordination through the medium of a nervous system is of later development. In the mammalian body both methods, as we have seen, are employed. Liver. — We do not usually regard the liver as furnishing an internal secretion. As a matter of fact, it does form two products within its cells — glycogen (sugar) and urea — which are subsequently given off to the blood for purposes of general nutrition or for elim- ination. The processes in this case fall under the general defini- tion of internal secretion, and, in fact, may be used to illustrate specifically the meaning of this term. The history of glycogen and urea has been considered. Internal Secretion of the Thyroid Tissues. — The most im- portant and definite outcome of the work on internal secretions has been obtained with the thyroids. Recent experimental work on this organ makes it necessary for us now to distinguish between the thyroid and the parathyroid tissues. The thyroids proper form two oval bodies lying on the sides of the trachea at its junction with the larynx. They have no ducts, and are composed of vesicles of different sizes, which are lined by a single layer of cuboidal epithe- lium and contain in their interior a material known as colloid. A number of histologists have traced the formation of this colloid to the lining epithelial cells, and have stated, moreover, that the vesicles finally rupture and discharge the colloid into the surrounding lym- phatic spaces. Accessory thyroids varying in size and number may be found along the trachea as far down as the heart. They possess a vesicular structure and no doubt have a function similar to that of the thyroid body. The parathyroids are, according to most authors, quite different structures. Four of these bodies are usually described, two on each side, and their positions vary somewhat in different animals or, indeed, in different individuals. In man the superior (or internal) parathyroids are found upon the posterior surface of the thyroid at the level of the junction of its upper with its middle third. They may be imbedded in the thyroid tissue. The inferior (or external) parathyroids lie near the lower margin of the thyroid on its posterior surface, and in some cases lower down on the sides of the trachea. * For general discussion, consult Starling, "Recent Advances in the Physiology of Digestion," Chicago, 1906. 836 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. The tissue has a structure quite different from that of the thy- roids, being composed of solid masses or columns of epithelial cells which are not arranged in vesicles and contain no colloid. Extirpation of the Thyroids and Parathyroids. — In 1856 Schiff showed that extirpation of the thyroids (complete thyroi- dectomy) in dogs is followed usually by the death of the animal in one to four weeks. The animal exhibits certain characteristic symp- toms, such as muscular tremors, which may pass into convulsions, cachexia, emaciation, and a condition of apathy. This result was confirmed by subsequent observers, but many exceptions were noted. Great interest was shown in these results, because on the surgical side reports were made showing that after complete removal of the thyroids in cases of goiter evil consequences might ensue, either acute convulsive attacks or chronic malnutrition. On the other hand, it became known that atrophy of the thyroids in the young is responsible for the condition of arrested growth and deficient mental development designated as cretinism, and in the adult the same cause gives rise to the peculiar disease of myxedema, character- ized by distressing mental deterioration, an edematous condition of the skin, loss of hair, etc. Schiff and others found that the evil results of complete thyroidectomy in dogs might be obviated by grafting pieces of the thyroid in the body, and this knowledge was quickly applied to human beings in cases of myxedema and cretinism with astonishingly successful results. Instead of grafting thyroid tissue it was found, in fact, that injection of extracts under the skin or better still simple feeding of thyroid material gave similar favorable results: the individuals recovered their normal appear- ance and mental powers.* Later Baumannf succeeded in isolating from the glands a substance designated as iodothyrin, which shows in large measure the beneficial influence exerted by thyroid extracts in cases of myxedema and parenchymatous goiter. This substance is characterized by containing a large amount of iodin (9.3 per cent, of the dry weight). It is contained in the gland in combina- tion with protein bodies, from which it may be separated by diges- tion with gastric juice or by boiling with acids. The Function of the Parathyroids. — Most of the results des- cribed above were obtained before the existence of the parathy- roid- was recognized. Early in the history of the subject it was recognized that complete removal of the thyroids proper inherbiv- orous animals (rats, rabbits) is not attended by a fatal result. Gley and others, however, proved thai if the parathyroids also are * For a general accounl of the development of the subject and the liter- ature see "Transactions of the Congress of American Physicians and Sur- Howell, Chittenden, Adami, Putnam, Kinnicutt, Osier), L897, and Jeandelize, 'In umsance thyroidienne et parathyroidienne, " Nancy, l(.)()2; also Vincent, 'Internal Secretions," etc., Lancet, A.ug. II and \x, 1906. Zeitschrifl f. phy iolog. Chemie," 21, 319, and 481, L896. SECRETION OF THE DUCTLESS GLANDS. 837 removed these animals die with the symptoms described in the case of dogs, cats, and other carnivorous animals. This result attracted attention to the parathyroids. Numerous experiments by Moussu, Gley, Vassale and Generale, and others * have seemed to show a marked difference between the results of thyroi- dectomy and parathyroidectomy. When the parathyroids alone are removed the animal dies quickly with acute symptoms, muscular convulsions (tetany), etc.; when the thyroids alone are removed the animal may survive for a long period, but develops a condition of chronic malnutrition, — a slowly increasing cachexia which may exhibit itself in a condition resembling myxedema in man. This distinction has been generally accepted, and it throws much light upon the discrepancy in the results obtained by some of the earlier observers. Complete thyroidectomy with the acutely fatal results usually described includes those cases in which both thyroids and parathyroids were removed, while probably many of the apparently negative results obtained after excision of the thyroids are expli- cable on the supposition that one or more of the parathyroids were left in the animal. It should be stated, however, that two recent observers, Vincent and Jolly, as the result of numerous experi- ments made upon different varieties of animals, throw some doubt upon these conclusions. They contend that in herbivorous animals fully half of those operated upon survive complete removal of all thyroid tissue, showing no evil symptoms except perhaps a di- minished resistance to infection. Carnivorous animals, on the con- trary, usually die after such an operation. In spite of such con- tradictory results in the hands of some observers the general opinion prevails that complete removal of the parathyroids is followed by acutely toxic results which develop rapidly and the most prom- inent symptom of which is muscular tetany. This tetany exhibits itself as fibrillar contractions of the muscles, a general muscular tremor, tonic and clonic spasms of the muscles or "intention spasms," that is, spasmodic or uncoordinated contractions follow- ing upon an effort to make a voluntary movement.* As is well known, similar symptoms are often observed under other condi- tions, infantile tetany, gastro-intestinal tetany, etc., and it has been suggested that in all such cases the initial difficulty may consist in the insufficiency of active parathyroid tissue. Several observers have reported that injections of extract of the parathy- roids cause the tetany to disappear without, however, protecting the animal from a fatal outcome, but the most striking results have been obtained by Macallum and Voegtlin.f These observers * For literature and Summary, see Bing, "Zentralblatt f. d. Physiol, u. Pathol, d. Stoffwechsels," 190S, Nos. 1 and 2. t Macallum and Voegtlin, "Johns Hopkins Hospital Bulletin," March, 1908. 838 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AXD SECRETION. find that injection or ingestion of solutions of calcium salts removes completely the symptoms of tetany and restores the animal to an apparently normal condition. They have obtained similar results upon human beings suffering from tetany as a result of unintentional removal of the parathyroids. The experimental evidence in the case of the parathyroids tends to support the view that their function consists in neutralizing in some way toxic substances formed elsewhere in the body, and that, therefore, after removal of these glands death occurs from the accumulation of such toxic bodies in the blood and tissues. Thus Macallum states that in animals in which tetany has developed as a conse- quence of extirpation of the parathyroids, bleeding and infusion of salt solution causes the tetany to disappear. The results quoted above in regard to the therapeutic value of calcium salts would seem, moreover, to connect the parathyroid function with the calcium metabolism and to relate the development of toxic substances with an insufficiency of calcium, but at present no precise statement can be made in regard to the way in which these bodies perform their very important function. The view that the parathyroids are simply immature thyroid tissue is still supported by some observers, being based chiefly on the his- tological assertion that after removal of the thyroids the para- thyroids may hypertrophy and show thyroid cysts containing colloidal material. Most observers, however, take the view outlined above, that the parathyroids have a functional signifi- cance essentially different from that of the thyroids, and that the parathyroids as they exist in the body are not simply undeveloped or immature thyroid tissue. At the same time it is becoming generally recognized that different as the functions of these two tissues may be, they are in some way correlated, and that the removal of oik; of them influences the activity of the other. The Function of the Thyroid. — According to the opinion of most writers on the subject, removal of the thyroid alone, Leaving, .-it Least, the external parathyroids uninjured, is followed by the development of a, state <>l chronic malnutrition which expresses itself finally in a condition of cachexia. Following a terminology sometimes used in medical Literature, this cachectic condition may be designated as "cachexia fh\Teopriva, " whereas the convulsive phenomena or tetany, formerly also described as nptom <>t' loss of I he thyroid, may be characterized as "tetania paral hyreopriva. " No adequate explantion has been furnished of the influence exercised by the thyroid on the nutrition of the body, h ie usually assumed I ha1 the thyroid cells form an internal secretion which is contained possibly in the colloid material found in the vesicles. This view assumes thai the thyroid forms SECRETION OF THE DUCTLESS GLANDS. 839 a specific hormone which acts as a chemical stimulus to other tissues, particularly those of the central nervous system. Some justification for this view is found in the effect of feeding thyroid tissue to normal individuals. There may be produced under these circumstances a condition which may be designated as hyperthyroidism, that is to say, the metabolism of the tissues is augmented as is shown by an increase in the excretion of nitrogen, carbon dioxid, and phosphoric acid, and by an increased con- sumption of oxygen, the heart-rate is also accelerated, and other evidences are given of an excitation of the nervous system. Simi- lar symptoms are observed in the pathological condition known as exophthalmic goiter, which is now usually explained as being due to a hyperthyroidism resulting from an hypertrophy of the thyroid tissue. As was stated above, Baumann isolated from the thyroid a peculiar substance, iodothyrin, which is characterized chemically by its large percentage of iodin, and physiologically by the fact that when used upon pa,tients suffering from a defi- ciency in functional activity of the thyroid (myxedema, goiter) it gives the same beneficial results as thyroid tissue itself. In the gland this iodothyrin is combined with protein to form a thyreo- globulin or thyreoprotein. There has been much discussion regarding the iodin constituent of the thyroid tissue. Extensive observations have shown that in some entirely healthy animals iodin is absent or is present only in traces, and in animals in which it is present the amount may vary greatly with the character of the food. Hunt gives the following table: Per cent, of iodin. Children's thyroid none. Maltese kid thyroid none. Guinea-pig thyroid 0.05 Dog thyroid 0.061 Cat thyroid 0.08 Sheep thyroid 0.176 Beef thyroid 0.25 Hog thyroid 0.33 Human (Wells) 0.236 Human (goitre) 0.04 Opinions in regard to the significance of the iodin have varied from the view, on the one hand, that it is an essential constituent of the physiologically active substance secreted by the gland, to the opposite extreme that it is an injurious substance which is bound and made innocuous by the thyroid cells. The balance of evidence seems to favor the first point of view,* and at present we may conclude that the iodin in some way intensifies the activity of the internal secretion of the thyroid. That it is abso- * For discussion and literature, consult Hunt, "Studies on Thyroid," "Hygienic Laboratory Bulletin," 1909, No. 47, Washington, D. C. 840 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. lutely necessary to this activity is rendered improbable by the fact that iodin-free thyroids appear still to exercise their normal influence upon metabolism, but administration of iodin in the food not only raises the iodin percentage in the gland but also increases proportionately the physiological activity of extracts of the tissue. Experiments show also that the known effects of thyroid extracts are greater in the iodin-rich than in the iodin- poor glands. Cyon's View of the Function of the Thyroid. — Cyon, in numerous publications, has advocated a different view of the function of the thyroids. These bodies have a very large vascular supply, and this author assumes that tin's area serves as a vascular shunt or flood-gate to protect mechanically the circulation in the brain. The dilatation of the thyroid area under con- ditions that threaten congestion of the brain is effected reflexly by means of the hypophysis cerebri and the vagi. For details of this mechanism and also of the supposed effect of the thyroid secretion on the irritability of the centers innervating the heart and blood-vessels see " Archives de physiologie, " 1898, p. 618. Thymus. — The physiology of the thymus gland is very obscure, indeed, practically nothing is known about its functions. Its prox- imity to the thyroids and parathyroids and its general similarity in origin would indicate that like them it may have some impor- tant specific influence upon metabolism, but physiological experi- iii nts so far have failed to discover what this influence is. Ac- cording to Verdun the thymus arises from the endothelial pouches belonging to the branchial clefts, chiefly from that of the third cleft. Formerly it was supposed to reach its maximal develop- ment at birth and subsequently to atrophy, being replaced by a growth of lymphoid and fatty tissues. More recently doubt has been thrown upon this belief. Several observers have stated that it continues to increase in size after birth until the appearance of puberty, and that true thymus tissue may persist throughout life. Siohr, in fact, insists that what has usually been taken as lymphoid tissue in the adult thymus is, in reality, epithelial or endothelial tissue which presumably has some specific function. The organ, in fact, seems to be an unusually labile structure. Deficient nutrition lead- to ;i rapid decline in weight. According to Jonson, chronic underfeeding in rabbits lor ;i, period of four weeks will reduce the weight to ;■!„ of its normal, and from this con- dition it recovers rapidly upon the restoration of a normal diet. On the physiological side, Abelous and Billard have stated thai extirpation of these glands in the frog is followed by the death of the animal, but later observers have failed to con- firm this result either up«m frogs or mammals, so that we musl believe that whatever their function may be if is not absolutely essential to the life of the organism. Removal of the SECRETION OF THE DUCTLESS GLANDS. 841 gland in young dogs (Basch) is said to cause a retarded growth of the bony tissues and to induce a condition resembling rickets. At the same time the peripheral nervous system shows an in- creased excitability as determined by the response of the nerves to galvanic stimulation. Injections of extract of the gland (Svehla) cause a fall of blood-pressure and some quickening of the heart-beat, but these effects are not specific. Unlike the thyroid and parathyroid glands, the thymus contains no iodin (Mendel). The only definite suggestion made regarding its influence is that there is some sort of reciprocal relationship between it and the re- productive glands. Castration (Henderson) causes a persistent growth and retarded atrophy of the thymus, while removal of the thymus (Paton) hastens the development of the testes.* Adrenal Bodies. — The adrenal bodies — or, as they are frequently called in human anatomy, the suprarenal capsules — belong to the group of ductless glands. It was shown first by Brown-Sequard (1856) that removal of these bodies is followed rapidly by death. This result has been confirmed by many experimenters, and so far as the observations go the effect of complete removal is the same in all animals. The fatal effect is more rapid than in the case of removal of the thyroids, death following the operation usually in two to three days, or, according to some accounts, within a few hours. The symptoms preceding death are great prostration, mus- cular weakness, and marked diminution in vascular tone. These symptoms resemble those occurring in Addison's disease in man, — a disease which clinical evidence has shown to be associated with pathological lesions in the suprarenal capsules. It has been ex- pected, therefore, that the results obtained from thyroid treatment of myxedema might be paralleled in cases of Addison's disease by the use of adrenal extracts, but so far these expectations have not been completely realized. Oliver f and Schaefer, and, about the same time, Cybulski and Szymonowicz,J discovered that this organ forms a peculiar substance that has a very definite physiological action, especially upon the circulatory system. They found that aqueous extracts of the medulla of the gland when injected into the blood of a living animal have a remarkable influence upon the heart and blood-vessels. If the vagi are intact, the adrenal extracts cause a very marked slowing of the heart beat together with a rise of blood- pressure. When the inhibitory fibers of the vagus are thrown out * References: Friedleben, "Die Physiologie der Thymusdriise, " 1858; Verdun, "Derives branchiaux chez les vertebres," 1898; Hammar, "Pfluger's Archiv, " ex., and "Anatom. Anzeiger, " xxvii.; Henderson, "Journal of Phys- iology," 1904, xxxi., 222; Stohr, "Beit. z. Anat. u. Entwick, Anatom.," Hefte, 1906, xxxi., 409; Jonson, "Archiv f. mik. Anat." 73, 390, 1909; Basch, " Jahrbuch f. Kinderheilkunde." 64, 1906, and 68, 1908. t "Journal of Physiology," 18, 230, 1895. % "Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie," 64, 97, 1896. S42 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. of action by section or by the use of atropin the heart rate is ac- celerated, -while the blood-pressure is increased sometimes to an extraordinary extent. These results are obtained with very small doses of the extracts, and only from extracts which include the medullary substance of the gland. The medullary cells contain a chromogen substance (chromaffin) which gives a yellow reaction with chromates. The physiological activity of the gland, so far as ilie effects on the circulatory system are concerned, seems to be proportional to the amount of chromaffin material in the medullary cells. Schaefer states that as little as 5V mgms. of the dried gland may produce a maximal effect upon a dog weigh- ing 10 kgms. The effects produced b}r such extracts are quite temporary in character. In the course of a few minutes the blood- pressure returns to normal, as also the heart-beat, showing that the substance has been destroyed in some way in the body, al- though where or how this destruction occurs is not known. Ac- cording to .Schaefer, the kidneys and the adrenals themselves are n< >t responsible for this prompt elimination or destruction of the active substance. Several observers have shown satisfactorily that the material producing this marked effect on the heart and blood-pressure is present in perceptible quantities in the blood of the adrenal vein, so that there can be but little doubt that it is a distinct internal secretion of the adrenal. Dreyer* has shown, moreover, that the amount of this substance in the adrenal blood is increased, judging from the physiological effects of its injection, by stimulation of the splanchnic nerve. Since this result was obtained independently of the amount of blood-flow through the gland. Dreyer makes the justifiable assumption that the adrenals possess secretory nerve libers. More recently it has been claimed by Schur and Wiesel Hint adrenalin is present in detectable amounts in the general circulation alter partial or complete nephrectomy, in rases of chronic nephritis and after prolonged muscular exercise. In such cases of excessive secretion the chromaffin substance in i he gland is apparently used up, since the reaction with chromates can no Longer be obtained. Abelf has succeeded in isolating a substance from the gland that pro- duce- the effecl on Mood-pressure and heart rale, and proposes for il the name epinephrin hydrate. He assigns to it, the formula C,0H13NO8 . III./), and describes it, as a peculiar, unstable, basic body. Salts of epinephrin may be obtained which when injected into the circulation cause the typical effects produced by injection of ex1 pacts of the gland. The acl ive principle of the »land has been prepared in crystalline form and named adrenalin (Takamine, Aldrich); its formula is * "American Journal of Physiology," 2, 203, 1899. f Abel, " Berichte d. 'lent. chem. Gesellschaft," 37, 368, 1904. SECRETION OF THE DUCTLESS GLANDS. 843 given as C9H13N03. This substance is much used practically in minor surgical operations as a hemostatic to check the flow of blood. The constriction of the blood-vessels seems to be due to a direct effect upon the walls of the vessels, either upon the musculature itself or upon the endings of the peripheral nerve fibers distributed to these muscles. That the effect is not entirely central is indicated by the fact that after destruction of the vasoconstrictor center and removal of the spinal cord injection of the extracts causes a rise of blood-pressure of 100 per cent, or more. Langley has called attention to the peculiar fact that the action of adrenal extracts or solutions of adrenalin on plain muscle resembles always the effect of stimulating the sympathetic nerves supplying the same tissue. It has been proposed, therefore, to use these solutions to determine whether or not given vascular areas, such as those of the brain or lungs, are provided with vasoconstrictor nerve fibers. When adrenalin is injected into a normal animal it may have an influence upon the nerve centers of the vasomotor nerves as well as upon the peripheral endings. Meltzer* has shown that mod- erate doses under such conditions may cause a dilatation, while in parts whose connection with the nerve center is destroyed only a constriction is obtained. Under normal conditions at least these extracts when injected into the circulation soon lose their effect. This fact may explain why injection of the extracts has failed to give permanent relief in animals from whom the adrenals had been removed or in human beings suffering from Addison's disease. Bearing in mind the results obtained in the case of thy- roidectomy, it has been suggested that grafts of the adrenals under the skin or into the peritoneal cavity may prove more effective. Results by this method have been chiefly negative, owing apparently to the fact that in such grafts the medullary substance, which con- tains the material that causes constriction of the blood-vessels, readily undergoes atrophy and absorption, but Busch and Van Bergen f report at least one successful transplantation of the adrenal (rabbit) in which the medulla persisted, and in which the grafted gland preserved the animal from the usual fatal result following total extirpation. It seems probable that if the method of grafting is perfected the procedure may prove effective as a therapeutical means in the treatment of Addison's disease. The Physiological Role of the Adrenals. — There seems to be no question that the medullary substance forms epinephrin or adre- nalin or some related compound which has a marked stimulating effect upon the tone of the blood-vessels and upon the heart, and that this material passes into the blood. The general view, there- * S. J. and Clara Meltzer, " American Journal of Physiology," 9, 252, 1903. t Busch and Van Bergen, " American Journal of Physiology," 1906, xv., 444. S4-4 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AXD SECRETION. fore, has been that one at least of the functions of the adrenals is the internal secretion of this material. It is assumed that its con- tinued formation is necessary to the maintenance of the normal metabolism of the muscular tissues either by a direct effect or indirectly by influencing the activity of the nerve centers. Removal of this secretion results in a marked loss of muscular tone and vigor, exhibited by the blood-vessels, the heart, and the skeletal muscles, and death follows rapidly. This general view is in accord with the facts so far as they are known, but it must be confessed that it goes somewhat beyond the facts. Another permissible, although less probable view is that the adrenals produce an antitoxic substance whose function is to neutralize or destroy certain (unknown) poi- sonous products of body metabolism. Removal or disease of the adrenals, on this theory, causes death because it allows these toxic products to accumulate. It should be noted that this sug- gestion regarding the functions of the adrenal glands refers only to the medullary portion, in which alone the substance caus- ing a rise of blood-pressure is found. Regarding the functions of the relatively large cortical portion of the gland we have no definite information, although some comparative observations indicate that it may have important specific relations to the growth of the body, particularly of the genital organs. Comparative observations show that the mammalian adrenals are charac- terized by the large development of the cortical substance.* Pituitary Body (Hypophysis). — This body is usually described as consisting of two parts — a large anterior lobe of distinct glandular structure and a much smaller posterior lobe of nervous origin and composed chiefly of neuroglia cells and fi Ikts. Embryologically the two lobes are entirely distinct. The anterior lobe arises from an invagination (Rathke's pouch) of the buccal ectoderm. A portion of this epithelium soon develops into a glandular structure belonging to the type of glands which have no excretory dud and which probably, therefore, form an internal secretion. The posterior lobe arises as an outgrowth from the floor of the third ventricle of the brain, the infundibulum, which comes into contact with the epithelial pouch forming the anterior lobe. The epithelial cells of the latter soon show a differentiation into two parts, one of which gives rise to the anterior lobe, while the other invests the body and neck of the rior or nervous lobe. To this latter the special name of the pars intermedia has been given. When fully formed the posterior lobe consists of two parts, the pars nervosa, composed of neuroglia cells and fibers and ependymal cells, and an investing of epithelial cells, derived from the buccal ectoderm and * Klliott and Tuckett, "Journal of Physiology," 34, 332, L906. SECRETION OF THE DUCTLESS GLANDS. 845 known as the pars intermedia* (see Fig. 289). The cells of the pars intermedia may also penetrate more or less into the sub- stance of the pars nervosa. Howellf and others have shown that extracts of the anterior lobe when injected intravenously have little or no physiological effect, while extracts of the posterior lobe, on the contrary, cause a marked rise of blood-pressure and slowing of the heart-beat. These effects resemble in general those obtained from adrenal extracts, but differ in some details. It was subsequently shown by Schafer and Herring! that extracts Fig. 289. — Mesial sagittal section through developing pituitary body of a human fetus (fifth month;. Drawing from a photograph. — (Herring.) a, Optic chiasma ; b, tongue- like process of epithelium ; c, third ventricle ; d, anterior lobe ; e, neck of posterior lobe ; /, epithelium surrounding neck ; g, epithelial cleft ; h, posterior lobe. made from the posterior lobe when injected into the blood cause a dilatation of the renal vessels and an increased secretion of urine. Evidence was thus obtained that the posterior lobe furnishes an internal secretion which has a specific effect upon the organs of circulation and upon the kidneys. Further work by Herring§ has made it very probable that this internal secretion * See Herring, "Quarterly Journal of Experimental Physiology, " 1, 121, 161, 1908. ** t "Journal of Experimental Medicine," 3, 245, 1898; also Schafer and Vincent, " Journal of Physiology, " 25, 87, 1899; and Herring, "Quarterly Journal of Experimental Physiology," 1, 261, 1908. X Schafer and Herring, "Philosophical Transactions, Royal Society," London, 1906, B. cxcix., 1. § Herring, loc. tit., and 1, 281, 1908. S46 PHYSIOLOGY OF DIGESTION AND SECRETION. is furnished by the epithelial cells of the pars intermedia and possibly also by the ependymal cells which belong properly to the posterior lobe. In the pars intermedia some colloid material is formed, and this is transported through the pars nervosa and excreted finally into the third -ventricle of the brain. There is some evidence also from histological appearances that this secre- tion is augmented after complete thyroidectomy, a fact which has led to the view that there is a functional relationship between this lobe of the pituitary body and the thyroid tissue. Physio- logical experiments upon the large glandular anterior lobe have given mostly negative results. Injections of its extracts cause no specific reaction. Indirect evidence, however, indicates that it has an important function connected in some way with body metabolism. On the pathological side, tumors or hypertrophies of the pituitary have been associated with the conditions known as acromegaly and gigantism. The former term applies to cases of disturbed nutrition in which there is abnormal growth, shown especially in the enlargement of the bones of the face and the extremities, while gigantism includes less distinctly pathological cases of overgrowth, particularly of the skeleton. That this abnormal nutrition is connected with a disturbance (hypertrophy) of the pituitary seems probable, but is not conclusively shown. It is assumed in these cases that it is the anterior lobe which is involved, and that, therefore, normally it controls in some way skeletal growth. A number of observers have attempted to remove all or a portion of the pituitary body, and in this way to arrive at a conception of its physiological importance. The results are somewhat conflicting. Yassale and Sacchi reported that com- plete removal of the pituitary body (hypophysectomy) results in the death of the animal with symptoms such as muscular tremors, apathy, and dyspnea, which resemble the effects of a complete thyroidectomy. Paulesco* has obtained more decisive results. Complete removal of the gland was followed by death in a short time twenty-four hours on the average. Mere separa- tion of the gland from the brain by division of the stalk gave an equally fatal result. As between the anterior and the posterior Lobes his experiments indicate thai the quickly fatal result is due to i he loss of 1 Ik; former. Ablation of the nervous lobe caused no appreciable evil effect. In this country the work of Paulesco has been confirmed by Eieford and Cushing,t so far, at least, as the fatal resull of a total hypophysectomy is concerned. Their animals soon after the operation exhibited a, condition of lethargy which passed rapidly into a coma that ended in death, but, the ; Paulesco, "Journal de Physiologie el .-.]*-, " Philosophical Transactions," JJ. excvii., 1904 ; also Vincent and , "Journal of Physiology, " L9O0, xxvii., xxxiv. SECRETION OF THE DUCTLESS GLANDS. 851 the secretion contains an enzyme which is necessary for the hydros ysis or oxidation of the sugar of the body and in the absence of this enzyme the sugar accumulates in the blood and is drained off through the kidney. Cohnheim* states that, while the juices ex- pressed from muscle and from pancreas have little effect upon sugar when taken separately, yet when combined they cause a marked disappearance (glycolysis) of sugar added to the mixture. The inference from this result is that the pancreas furnishes a substance which activates the glycolytic enzyme or enzymes of the muscle and thus makes possible the physiological consumption of sugars in the body. Since the pancreas extracts do not lose this property upon boiling it is evident that the activating substance is not an enzyme, but a body of a more stable character (hormone) . Other investigators adopt an entirely different view of the relation of the pancreas to carbohydrate metabolism. They believe that the internal secretion of the pancreas regulates in some way the output of sugar from the liver (and other sugar-producing organs). In the absence of this secretion the liver gives off its glycogen as sugar too rapidly, the sugar contents of the blood are thereby increased (hyperglycemia) above normal, and the excess passes out in the urine. Kidney. — Tigerstedt and Bergman f state that a substance may be extracted from the kidneys of rabbits which when injected into the body of a living animal causes a rise of blood-pressure. They get the same effect from the blood of the renal vein. They conclude, therefore, that a substance, for which they suggest the name "rennin," is normally secreted by the kidney into the renal blood, and that this substance causes a vasoconstriction. Other observers claim that the kidneys furnish an important internal secretion that affects the metabolism. The absence of this secretion after complete nephrectomy leads to the production of uremia.! * Cohnheim, " Zeitschrift f. physiolog. Chemie," 39, 336, 1903; also 1904. f " Skandinavisches Archiv. f. Physiologie," 8, 223, 1898 ; see also Brad- ford, "Proceedings of the Royal Society," 1892. J Suner, " Zentralblatt f. d. ges. Physiol, u. Path, des Stofferechsel," 1907, ii., 3. SECTION VIII. NUTRITION AND HEAT PRODUCTION AND REGULATION. CHAPTER XLVIL GENERAL METHODS-HISTORY OF THE PROTEIN FOOD. Under the head of nutrition or general metabolism we include usually all those changes that occur in our foodstuffs from the time that they are absorbed from the alimentary canal until they are eliminated in the excretions. In many of these processes the oxj'gen absorbed from the lungs takes a most important part, and the changes directly due to this element, the physiological oxidations of the body, can not be separated from the general metabolic phe- nomena of the tissues. As was said in another place, the respiratory history of oxygen ceases after this element has reached the tissues; its subsequent participation in the chemical changes of the organ- ism forms an integral part of the nutritional processes. These latter processes are varied and complex and only partially understood. For the sake of simplicity in presentation it is convenient to con- sider separately each of the so-called foodstuffs, — the proteins, carbohydrates, fats, water, and inorganic salts, — and attempts to trace its nutritive history from the time it is absorbed into the blood until it is eliminated from the body in the form of excretory products. Before undertaking this description it is desirable to full attention to certain general methods and conceptions that have been developed in connection with this part of physiology. Nitrogen Equilibrium. — Among our main foodstuffs the pro- teins (and albuminoids) are characterized by containing nitrogen. After this materia] is metabolized in the body the nitrogen is elim- inated in various forms, chiefly in the urine, but to a smaller ex- tent in the feces and sweat. In the feces, moreover, there may be presenl some undigested protein which, although taken with the food, has never really entered the body. It is evident that the urine, feces (and sweat) may be collected during a given period and analyzed to determine their contents in nitrogen. The sweat is» 852 GENERAL METHODS HISTORY OF PROTEIN FOOD. 853 usually neglected except in observations upon conditions in which muscular activity has been a prominent feature. As a rule, the amount of nitrogen is determined by some modification of the Kjel- dahl method. In principle this method consists in heating the material to be analyzed with strong sulphuric acid. The nitrogen is thereby converted to ammonia, which is distilled off and caught in a standardized solution of sulphuric acid. By titration the amount of ammonia can be determined, and from this the amount of nitrogen is estimated. Nitrogen forms a definite percentage of the protein molecule (about 16 per cent.) ; so that if the weight of nitro- gen is multiplied by 6.25 the weight of protein from which it is de- rived is obtained. If, on the other hand, the nitrogen is determined in the food eaten during the period of the experiment it is evident that a balance may be struck which will determine whether the body is receiving or losing nitrogen. If the balance is even the body is in nitrogen equilibrium — that is, it is receiving in the food as much protein nitrogen as it is metabolizing and eliminating in the excreta. If there is a plus balance in favor of the food it is evident that the body is laying on or storing protein tissue, while if the balance is minus, the body must be losing protein. During the period of growth, in convalescence, etc., the body does store protein, and under these conditions the balance is in favor of the food nitrogen. But throughout adult life under normal conditions our diet is so regulated by the appetite that a nitrogen equilibrium is maintained through long periods. Under experimental condi- tions, involving, for instance, a special diet, it often becomes neces- sary to make the analyses for nitrogen in order to determine whether or not the individual is losing or gaining protein or is in equilibrium. It is important also to bear in mind that nitrogen or protein equilibrium may be established at different levels. If, for instance, a man is in nitrogen equilibrium on a diet containing 10 gms. of nitrogen, what will happen if the protein in this diet is doubled ? Our experience teaches us that the extra 10 gms. of nitrogen or 62.5 gms. of protein is not stored in the body indefinitely. As a matter of fact, the extra protein is metabolized in the body and nitrogen equilibrium becomes established at a higher level. Where- as under the first condition 62.5 gms. of protein were eaten and 62.5 gms. of protein were lost from the body, either in the form of nitrog- enous excreta or in the feces as undigested protein, under the second condition 125 gms. of protein are eaten and 125 gms. of pro- tein are lost. The total mass of protein tissue in the body may remain the same, or if any increase takes place at the beginning of the change in diet it soon ceases. Experimentally it is found that there is a certain low limit of protein which just suffices to maintain nitrogen equilibrium, and between this level and the capacity of 854 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. the body to digest and absorb protein food nitrogen equilibrium may be maintained upon any given amount of protein. Carbon Equilibrium and Body Equilibrium. — The term car- bon equilibrium is sometimes used to describe the condition in which the total carbon of the excreta (in the carbon dioxid, urea, etc.) is balanced by the carbon of the food. It is possible that an individual may be in nitrogen equilibrium and yet be losing or gaining in weight, since, although the consumption of proteins may just be covered by the proteins of the food, the comsumption of non-protein material, particularly the fats of the body, may be greater than the supply furnished by or manufactured from the food. An animal may lose or gain in carbon when his nitrogen supply is in equilib- rium. In the same way under special circumstances we may speak of a water equilibrium or a salts equilibrium, although these terms are not generally used. An adult under normal conditions lives so as to maintain a general body equilibrium; his ingesta of all kinds are balanced by the corresponding excretions, and the individual maintains a practically constant body-weight. Complete Balance Experiments — Respiration Chamber. — According to the statements made in the last paragraph, it is obvious that if the analytical work is properly done, an exact balance may be drawn between the proteins, fats, and carbohydrates eaten as food and the proteins, fats, and carbohydrates destroyed in the body as represented by the nitrogen and carbon contained in the excreta. Complete experiments of this kind were attempted first by Voit * and Pettenkofer, to whose work much of our fundamental knowledge is due. In the experiments of these authors, made upon men as well as animals, the total nitrogen of the urine and feces was determined and the total quantity of C02 given off from the lungs was estimated. This last determination was made possible by placing the individual in a specially constructed chamber or respi- ration apparatus. Air was drawn through this room by means of a pump. The total quantity of air passing through the room was measured by a gasometer and definite fractions were drawn off from time to time, which were analyzed for C02. From the figures thus obtained it was possible to estimate the entire C02 given off during the period of observation. Knowing the total nitrogen and carbon eliminated, it was possible to estimate the amount of protein and fat or carbohydrate destroyed in the body. From the nitrogen the quantity of protein metabolized was obtained by multipying by b\25, as explained above. If then the carbon belonging to the amount of protein metabolized was deducted from the total carbon excreta, what was left represented either fa1 or carbohydrate burnt in the body, and, knowing the * See Hermann's "Handbuchder Physiologic, " vol. vi., 1881. GENERAL METHODS HISTORY OF PROTEIN FOOD. 855 amount of these materials taken in the diet, it was possible to ascertain whether the corresponding amount of carbon had all been excreted. By experiments of this kind a nearly perfect balance may be struck between the income and the outgo of the body. Absolute accuracy is not sought for, since the materials eaten vary somewhat in composition and some little of the carbon or nitrogen excreted is found in the secretions from the skin, the saliva, etc, which are not usually examined. More recent experiments made in this country under the direc- tion of Atwater* have attempted to balance not only the material income and outgo of the body during a given period, but also the income and outgo of energy. For this purpose the individuals ex- perimented upon were placed in a very carefully constructed respi- ration chamber so that their expired air could be analyzed as well as the urine and feces. The chamber, however, was also arranged to act as a calorimeter (see p. 907) by means of which the heat given off by the person could be measured. The heat value of the diet being known, it is possible in this way to ascertain whether or not this theoretical amount of heat is actually given off from the body. Atwater's respiration chamber is described as a respiration calorim- eter; some of the results obtained from its use are referred to later on. The Effect of Non-protein Food on Nitrogen Equilibrium. — By use of the methods referred to above the general influence of the non-protein foods (fats, carbohydrates, albuminoids) upon the protein consumption of the body has been made evident. An ani- mal may be brought into nitrogen equilibrium on protein food alone, the amount of protein required being relatively large. If now non-protein foodstuffs are added to the diet it is found that the amount of protein necessary to maintain nitrogen equilibrium may be reduced correspondingly. With reference to the consumption of protein in the body the non-protein foods are all protein-spar ers, and herein lies one great peculiarity of their nutritional value. On a mixed diet of protein and non-protein food the proportion of the latter may be increased and that of the former decreased to a marked extent without breaking down nitrogen equilibrium — that is, without causing a loss of protein tissue from the body. This fact is explained by the consideration that in our body the food fulfils two great functions. First, it furnishes the material for the formation of new living matter or the replacement of the loss of this matter that is continually going on; second, it furnishes a supply of energy for the work done by the various cells, the contraction of the muscle, the secretion of the gland, the discharges of the nerve cells, etc. This second function, the energy requirement, is met by any of the three energy-yielding food-stuffs, carbohydrates, fats, or proteins, especially, as we shall * Atwater, Bulletins 45, 63, 69, United States Department of Agriculture. S56 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. find, by the carbohydrates. For the first, function protein (or its split-products) is absolutely needed, and perhaps is alone needed. In any event, if the supply of non-protein is sufficiently large, then the amount of protein can be lowered to a certain irreducible minimum which is required for purposes of genuine assimilation, that is, the construction of living material. The Nutritive History of the Protein Food. — The digestive changes undergone by protein and its subsequent absorption have been described in the section on Digestion. It will be remembered that the products of protein digestion are absorbed mainly into the blood-vessels of the intestine, and therefore must pass through the liver before reaching the general circulation. It will also be remem- bered that we are as yet ignorant of the precise form in which these products enter the portal blood. This deficiency in our knowledge constitutes a serious obstacle to a satisfactory explanation of the nutritional history of the protein. Three general views have been advanced concerning the ultimate fate of the absorbed material. In two of these theories it is assumed that the digested material is all synthesized into a new protein, during or after absorption, being converted into what we might call a body protein charac- teristic of the animal. Although it is not specifically stated the assumption seems to be that this body protein is serum-albumin or, at least, one of the blood-proteins. Accepting this general assumption, one theory, advocated by Pfluger, supposes that before undergoing physiological oxidation all of this absorbed material is built up into the living protoplasm of the various tissues and then undergoes the characteristic metab- olism (Vatabolism or disassimilation) of thai tissue. The second theory, advanced by Voit, assumes that some of the absorbed material is assimilated to form living protoplasm, so far as this is necessary to replace the wastes of the tissue or to provide new material for growth. The portion of the absorbed protein tli.it subserves this function is designated as tissue protein. It is obvious l hut this function cannot be replaced by the non-protein — th:it is, the uon-nitrogenous— foodstuffs. The larger portion of the absorbed material, however, after distribution to the tissues is dest rayed, with liberation of heat, under the influence of the activity of the living cells, but without actually becoming transformed into living matter. The cells act toward this material as the yeast cells do toward the sugar thai they decompose into alcohol and carbon dioxid. The portion of the protein that undergoes this fate i- de ignated a the circulating protein, on the hypothesis that it enter- the circulating liquids of the body, the blood, and lymph. The third general point of view represents perhaps the trend of modern investigation, it starts from the belief now generally GENERAL METHODS — HISTORY OF PROTEIN FOOD. 857 accepted that the digestive processes do not stop at the peptone stage, but result in splitting the protein molecule more or less completely into its constituent amino bodies, or into a mixture of such amino bodies and polypeptids. From this group of split products, none of which is of sufficient complexity to be desig- nated as a protein, some protein is reconstructed by processes of synthesis taking place in the wall of the intestine or in the liver. That this is a possibility, that, in other words, the body may build its own prorein out of the split products of a complete pancreatic hydrolysis, has been demonstrated by the work of Loewi and others.* Dogs fed on the split products of a pancreatic digestion, together with sufficient carbohydrates and fats, may be kept in nitrogenous equilibrium or may even store up protein in the body. It is interesting to know that the split products of pro- tein obtained by complete hydrolysis with boiling acids cannot be utilized in this way by the body. The end products of pancreatic and acid hydrolysis of protein are very similar, but evidently the latter either goes too far or results in the production of sec- ondary reactions which unfit the split products for synthesis by the tissues of the body. It is possible, as was suggested first by Abderhalden, that in the normal digestion of protein in the body some polypeptids remain which serve as a sort of nucleus for the reconstruction of the body protein. Any way, the view that we are now describing assumes that the protein material of the food is first broken down quite completely in digestion and then a new body protein is reconstructed from some of this material. In other words, out of the various pieces into which the food-protein is split by the processes of digestion a certain number are united by synthetic processes to form the special body-protein of the animal. The balance of the amino bodies not thus used is of value to the body only as a source of energy, but not for tissue building. The nitrogen in them is useless to the body and con- sequently it is promptly split off, probably in the liver, as ammonia, which is then converted to urea and excreted. The organic acid group left behind is important as a source of energy. It can be oxidized with the production of heat, or if the food is in excess of the energy requirements of the body, it may be converted to glyco- gen or fat and stored as a reserve. On this view, therefore, one would not speak of circulating protein as contrasted with tissue- protein. So much of the food protein as is not resynthesized into tissue protein for the construction of tissue, is used or destroyed for purposes of energy without again passing into the protein form. * For discussion and literature, consult "Luthje in Ergebnisse der Physi- ologie," vii., 1908. 858 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. Folin* has called attention to the fact that the proportions of the different nitrogen compounds in the urine vary with the amount of protein food. Upon an average diet containing 16 to 17 gms. of nitrogen (100 to 106 gms. of usable protein) the urea forms S7 to 88 per cent, of the total nitrogen of the urine, while when the protein intake is reduced to 3 or 4 gms. of nitrogen the urea forms only 61 to 62 per cent, of the total nitrogen of the urine. On the other hand, the creatinin and the purin bodies (uric acid, xanthin, etc.) are not diminished in amount with a decrease in the protein food. He suggests, therefore, that the hitter bodies, creatinin and purin bases, arise from the breaking down of the living tissues, the catabolism or wear and tear of the living machinery, and may be taken as an index of the extent of this metabolism. The urea, on the other hand, represents in part that portion of the protein food which, from the present point of view, is not used for construction of living matter, but which acts simply as an energy food. The amount of urea, there- fore, while a reliable index of the weight of protein destroyed in the body, is not an index, as was formerly supposed of the amount of living tissue protein broken down, since a portion of it may arise from the food-protein as described above. The Amount of Protein Necessary for Normal Nutrition. — As was stated above, nitrogen equilibrium may be maintained on different amounts of protein food. It is important, from a scientific and from an economic standpoint, to determine the low limit for this equilibrium and to ascertain whether, for the purpose of the best as well as the most economical nutrition, this low limit is as good as or preferable to a higher amount of protein in the diet. Examination of the dietaries of civilized races shows that, on the average, 100 to 120 gms. of protein are used daily by an adult man. Voit gives 118 gms. of protein as the average daily consumption. A variable portion of this amount passes into the feces in undigested form, but we may assume that about 100 to 105 gms. are absorbed and actually metabolized in the body. If we take into account the weight of the body, this amount of protein may bo estimated as equivalent in round numbers to 1.5 gms. of pTot fin for 0.23 gm. nitrogen) per kilogram of body- weight. In recent years serious attempts have been made to lain how low this daily quota of protein may be reduced without destroying nitrogen equilibrium or injuring the effective- ness of the body for muscular or mental work. Siven was able for short periods to red u re his daily diet of protein to as little as 0.5 gm. (0.08 gm. N.) per kilo of body weight, but probably the iiio.-I important experiments of this kind were those carried out * Folin, "American Journal of Physiology," xiii., l.r>, <>(>, and 1 17, L905. GENERAL METHODS HISTORY OF PROTEIN FOOD. 859 by Chittenden.* In this work the experiments were continued over long periods of time, and were made upon three different groups of men, five university teachers, a detail of thirteen men from the Hospital Corps of the Army, and eight university students classed as athletes. The general result of the investigation showed that the body can be maintained in protein equilibrium and in a normal state of efficiency upon a diet containing only 30 to 50 gms. of protein per day, according to the weight of the individual — or, expressed in more general terms, the daily quota of protein per kilo of weight may be reduced from 1.5 gms. (0.23 gm. N.) to about one-half, that is, 0.75 gm. of protein or 0.12 gm. of nitrogen per kilo. This general result has been confirmed on a large scale by the studies made by McCabef of the metabolism of the Bengalis of India. He finds that the average Bengali metabolizes in his body, so far as may be judged from the nitrogen excreted in the urine, only about 37.5 gms. of protein daily, corresponding to a consumption per kilo of 0.7 gm. of protein or 0.113 gm. of nitrogen. A corresponding average amount of protein was, of course, eaten daily, and on this low protein diet they exist in apparent health. Rubner % also empha- sizes the fact that milk, which forms the sole diet of the infant, is & protein poor food. The usual daily diet of the adult has a heat value of from 2400 to 3000 calories (see p. 900). Of this total heat value the protein food in the diets usually recommended forms about 15 to 20 per cent. In milk, however, according to Kubner's estimates, the protein constitutes only about 10 per cent, of the total heat value. As the result of these and similar investigations, the practical question presents itself as to what constitutes the optimum daily quota of protein. If the body can be kept in good condition upon 0.75 gm. per kilo per day, will an ingestion of more than this, say twice as much, prove injurious or beneficial or indifferent to the body? Outside its hygienic aspect the question is important from an economical standpoint, since the proteins are the most expensive foods, and in the feeding of large masses of individuals — armies, schools, asylums, etc. — it is not desirable to waste money on protein food if it is not needed. The full and satisfactory answer to this question must be deferred until more experience is obtained. The report upon the Bengalis, noted above, would seem at first to constitute a satisfactory demonstration of the practicability of a low protein * Consult Chittenden, "Physiological Economy in Nutrition," New York, 1905, for discussion and literature. f McCabe, " The Metabolism of the Bengalis, Calcutta," 1908. (Scientific Memoirs, Medical Department Government of India, No. 34.) % Rubner, "Das Problem des Lebensdauer," 1908; Cohnheim, "Die Physiologie der Verdauung u. Ernahrung," 1908. 860 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. diet, but McCabe states that the Bengali is inferior physically to the average European, and is particularly deficient in capacity for muscular work, and he is inclined to attribute this inferiority to the diet. Moreover, the Bengali is quite susceptible to kidney troubles, a fact which seems to destroy one prediction often made by those who advocate a low protein diet, namely, that the smaller amount of work thus thrown on the kidneys would result in a diminution of diseases of the kidney. The newer conceptions in regard to the digestion and nutritive history of the protein foods certainly seem to favor the adoption of a low protein diet. If protein is eaten in excess of the real assimilation needs of the tissues, all the excess, so far as we can see, might just as well be substituted by carbohydrate or by carbohydrate and fat. The excess nitrogen thus eaten appears to be so much useless ballast which the body very promptly gets rid of. The uncertain point, however, is what constitutes the assimilation need of the tissues. The experiments given above would place this need very low, according to the lowest estimate, at about 5 per cent, of the total energy value of the food. That is to say, if the daily diet contains heat energy equivalent to 2400 calories, only 5 per cent, of this, 120 calories, needs to be in the form of protein, an estimate which would bring the protein to about 30 gms. daily. Against this line of reasoning it may be urged, in the first place, that our positive knowledge of the history of protein in the body is too incomplete to justify its application in a wholesale way to such an important matter as the daily diet. Serious blunders have been made in the past, notably in the nutritive employment of gelatin, by a premature application of incomplete knowledge. Secondly, it must be remembered that mankind, left to the guidance of the natural appetites and the eliminating influence of natural selec- lion, has always, when possible, adopted the high protein level of 90 to 100 gms. per day. Indeed, the uniformity with which this level has been unconsciously maintained is a striking fact. Among the rich as well as the poor, and in races very differently placed ae regards quantity of available food, substantially the same amounl of protein (80 to LOO gms.) is consumed daily by each Individual. The element of the diet which varies most widely, iinlicim points oul in an interesting discussion of this ques- tion, is, on the contrary, the non-protein, particularly the carbo- hydrate material. Those who arc obliged to do much muscular work to earn a Living or for the sake of pleasure (spoils, athletics) add to their daily quota of protein an excess of carbohydrate food to furnish the requisite energy. On the contrary, those whose daily Life requires bul little muscular exertion, cut down the carbo- hydrates and fats, and make their diet relatively but not abso- GENERAL METHODS HISTORY OF PROTEIN FOOD. 861 lutely richer in protein. That mankind has made a mistake in adopting the higher protein level can hardly be claimed on the basis of our present knowledge. We must be content to await until the matter is tested more completely on a larger scale or until our knowledge of the details of protein metabolism is more satisfactory. The Intermediate Stages in Protein Metabolism. — The urea found in the urine and in lesser amounts in the sweat and other secretions may arise in two general ways: 1. As an end-product of the digestive hydrolysis of the protein food. As was explained above, the protein material is split by the successive actions of the pepsin, trypsin, and erepsin into products which no longer give the biuret reaction for protein. As a result of this process much of the nitrogen appears in the form of ammonia, monamino-acids, and the so-called diamino-bodies, such as arginin, and we may suppose that in these forms some of it is carried to the liver. In this organ the ammonia, as ammonia salts, is transformed into urea. The monamino-acids, some of them, at least, are deamidized, that is, their NH2 group is split off as ammonia which then is like- wise converted to urea. The organic acid radicles left after removal of the NH2 group may subsequently be oxidized through various stages to carbon dioxid and water, or they may be synthesized to form a carbohydrate body or possibly a fat, and thus be kept temporarily as storage, although their eventual fate is to suffer oxidation to carbon dioxid and water. Regarding the diamino- compounds like arginin it is known that when this substance is injected subcutaneously its nitrogen is excreted for .the most part if not entirely as urea. Since Kossel and Dakin have shown that the liver especially contains a hydrolytic enzyme, arginase, which is capable of splitting off the guanidin residue of arginin to form urea, we may assume that the arginin formed during protein diges- tion actually undergoes this fate. The process is represented by the following equation : NHCf the Nuclcoprotcins.—Nudeo- proteins are taken in our fond and likewise are found in the tissues of the body. Their metabolism, so far as the nuclein portion is concerned, gives rise to the formation of uric acid and the purin bases. It will be remembered that in the urine we find some uric acid and some xanthin and hypoxanthin. In the feces small amounts may be detected of xanthin, hypoxanthin, adenin, and guanin. These bodies all contain the purin group or radicle (see GENERAL METHODS — HISTORY OF PROTEIN FOOD. 863 p. 817) and are closely related chemically. By hydrolytic processes outside the body one may obtain their purin bases from nucleins or nuclein acids, and inside the body the metabolic processes give similar products. The history of the intermediary metabolism may be outlined as follows: When nucleoprotein is eaten the nuclein is split off apparently by the action of the pepsin, the protein portion then undergoing the digestive and metabolic changes already described. The nuclein is not acted upon further by the pepsin or trypsin, except that under the influence of the latter it is rendered soluble possibly by an act of hydration.* Once within the body the nuclein is submitted to the action of a series of enzymes as follows: Under the influence of nuclease it is split with the production of some of the purin bases, adenin, guanin. The amino- purins so far as they are formed are converted to the corresponding oxypurins by the action of a deamidizing enzyme. According to Jones two such enzymes may occur in the body, namely, adenase and guanase. Their action takes place as follows: C5H5N5 + H20 = C8Hr Nul ril i<>n," Philadelphia, L906. GENERAL METHODS HISTORY OF PROTEIN FOOD. 865 which are formed in digestion, but are not reconstructed into protein. Thus one gram of alanin has a heat value of 4.37 cal. and may be considered as giving one gram of lactic acid after removal of its NH2 group — one gram of lactic acid has a heat value of 3.66 cal., hence in the process 0.7 cal. of heat has been liberated, a loss of about 16 per cent. Nutritive Value of Albuminoids. — The albuminoid most fre- quently occurring in food is gelatin. It is derived from collagen of the connective tissue. Collagen of bones or of connective tissue takes up water when boiled and becomes converted into gelatin. We eat gelatin, therefore, in boiled meats, soups, etc., and, besides, it is frequently employed directly as a food in the form of table gelatin. Collagen has the following percentage composition: C, 50.75 per cent.; H, 6.47; N, 17.86; 0, 24.32; S, 0.6. It resembles the protein molecule closely in percentage composition and, in fact, is now classified as one of the varieties of protein. It would seem that the tissues might use it as they do protein for the for- mation of new protoplasm. Experiments, however, have demon- strated clearly that this is not the case when it is employed without other protein food. Animals fed upon albuminoids together with fats and carbohydrates do not maintain nitrogen equilibrium. The final result of such a diet would be continued loss of weight and malnutrition and death. Some light is thrown upon the inability of the gelatin to act as a tissue former by a considera- tion of the split products formed from it in hydrolytic cleavage. While it yields a number of the products usually given by the proteins, there are others which are lacking, such as cystein (thioaminopropionic acid), tyrosin (oxyphenylaminopropionic acid), serin (oxyaminopropionic acid), and tryptophan (indol- aminopropionic acid). On the hypothesis that proteins during digestion are normally split more or less completely into then constituent parts, and that the characteristic body-protein of the animal is reconstructed synthetically by a new arrangement of these groups, it is apparent that the gelatin, when used without protein, may fail to furnish some of the groupings necessary to such a synthesis. Gelatin is readily digested, gelatoses and gelatin peptones and eventually some split products being formed; these are absorbed and oxidized in the bod}*, with the formation of C02, H,0, and urea. Gelatin serves, then, as a source of energy to the body in the same sense as do carbohydrates and fats. When any one of these three substances is used in a diet, the proportion of protein necessary for the maintenance of nitrogen equilibrium may be reduced greatly. Actual experiments have shown that gelatin is more efficacious than either fats or carbo- hydrates in protecting the protein in the body. The relative 55 S66 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. value of fats, carbohydrates, and gelatin in protecting protein from destruction in the bod)' is illustrated by an experiment report ed by Yoit: A dog weighing 32 kgms. was fed alternately upon protein and sugar, protein and fat, and protein and gelatin, with the following result: Nourishment (Gms.) Calculated Destruction of Meat. Gelatin. Fat. Sugar. Flesh in Body (Gms.). 400 200 450 400 250 439 400 200 356 This greater efficacy of the gelatin is doubtless connected with its nitrogen-containing groups and would indicate an ability to partly replace protein material in its assimilative functions. Practically, however, the use of gelatin in diets is restricted by its unpalatableness when employed in large quantities. Whatever may be the physiological cause of this peculiarity, there seems to be no doubt that when used largely in the diet both animals and men soon develop such an aversion to it that it is necessary to dis- continue its use. A number of observers have attempted to determine experimentally just how far the protein of the food may be replaced by gelatin without causing a loss of body-protein. Munk states, from experiments upon dogs, that when about six- sevenths of the nitrogen necessary to maintain equilibrium was given in the form of gelatin, the animal could be kept in nitrogen equilibrium for a few days at least. Murlin,* in more careful experiments, has shown that if abundant carbohydrate food is used a dog may be kept in nitrogen equilibrium at or near the fasting level on a diet in which two-thirds of the protein (meat) nitrogen is substituted by gelatin nitrogen. Kaufniannf claims to have kept himself in nitrogen equilibrium for a short time upon a diet in which no protein was contained, all the nitrogen being supplied in the form of gelatin, together with small amounts of the amino-acids, which are lacking in the gelatin molecule (cystin, tyrosin, tryptophan). The history of gelatin as a food is very interesting and, Indeed, instructive, since it Berves or should serve as n warning againsl a premature application of the results of scientific investigation. A condensed account of the subject is given l is deprived in succession of its carbon atoms by- oxidation, with the formation of simple fatty acids, but little positive evidence has been obtained of intermediate products. Perhaps the most significant fact known bearing upon this point is that under conditions which involve a large destruction of fat in the body, as in starvation, fevers, and especially in diabetes, /9-oxybutyric acid together with aceto-acetic acid and acetone are excreted in the urine. These three substances are designated as the acetone bodies, and their appearance in the urine makes the condition known as acetonuria. The oxybutyric acid may be regarded as the source of the other two. as may be inferred from their formulas. ,5-oxybutyric acid = CH3CHOHCH2COOH. By oxidation this yields aceto-acetic acid. CH3COCH2COOH, and this by loss of CO, is converted to acetone, CH,C~OCHr The evidence seems to show that the oxybutyric acid arises from the fats, and it represents probably one of the simpler fatty acids formed in the intermediate metabolism of the fats. There is some indication also that the liver cells, along with their numerous other functions, are concerned in some of the intermediary stages of fat metabolism. Under abnormal conditions, such as phos- phorus-poisoning, the fat of the adipose tissues is transported to the liver, and it is suggested that this transportation may be a normal process, that before the neutral fat is actually oxidized or is converted into the phosphorus-containing fats of the tissues (lecithin, etc.), it is acted upon by the liver, possibly in the way of desaturating the fatty acids, since the fat actually found in the liver contains more unsaturated fatty acids than the storage fat of the adipose tissues. Whatever may be the real nature of the connection, both microscopic and chemical evidence indicates that the liver is concerned in some phase of fat metabolism.* According to the experiments of Knoopf the oxidation of the fatty acids begins with the carbon occupying 1 1 1 * - beta position. In the case of butyric acid this would result al once in the formation of /i-oxybutyr ic acid. In the Case of higher fatty acids containing an even number of carbon atoms a Bunilar process would result in the formation, first, of butyric acid and then of the '-oxybutyric acid. The series of oxidations of caproie acid may be represented as follows: % (\ll;uil,r!l,rooii + o = C,H,CHOHCHjCOOH Caproie acid. Oxycaproic acid. • 1 1 / 1 1 o 1 1 u i I zoo 1 1 f- 6o - cjicooii i 2HaO + 2C02 Butyric acid. CH,CHjCH,COOH t 0 - CH,CHOHCH,,COOH Butyric acid. /9-oxybutyric acid, * For discussion and facts, sec Leathes, "Lancet," Feb. 27, L909. 1 Knoop, " Ilofmeistcr's licit rage, " 6, 150. L904. X Lusk, "Archives of internal Medicine/' Feb., 1909. CARBOHYDRATES AND FAT. 877 On this view, fatty acids with an odd number of carbon atoms cannot yield /3-oxybutyric acid. Some of the amino-acids derived from the hydrolysis of protein may, however, serve as a source of this acid, leucin, for example, so that in this way the protein of the food as well as the fat may be responsible for the presence of this acid in diabetes. The inability of the tissues to oxidize sugar in the case of diabetes is associated in many instances with a loss of the power of oxidizing the /3-oxybutyric acid, but the reason for this relationship is not clear. To the extent that the /3-oxybutyric acid is not burnt and not excreted, it accumulates in the body and produces a condition of acidosis which, in turn, is responsible for the development of diabetic coma. Origin of the Body Fat. — The views upon the origin of body fat have undergone a number of changes in the last fifty or sixty years, illustrating in an interesting way how development of our experimental methods leads often at first to half-truths which are corrected later by more extensive work. Dumas and others (1840) held to the natural view that the fat of the body originates directly from the fat of the food. Liebig, applying his more exact methods, demonstrated that in some cases at least this source is insufficient to account for all the fat. The fat yielded by the milk of a cow for instance, may be greater in quantity than the fat contained in the food. He also pointed out that the fat ox each species of animal is more or less peculiar, the fat of the sheep having a higher melting point than pork fat, and both differing in composition from the fat taken as food. "In hay or the other fodder of oxen no beef suet exists, and no hog's lard can be found in the potato refuse given to swine." He was led to attribute the source of body fat chiefly to the carbohydrate food, and this belief agreed well with the experience of agriculturists as to the use of such foods in fatten- ing animals for market. This view, in turn, was displaced by the theory of Voit, supported by elaborate feeding experiments. Voit believed that the fat of the body is formed mainly or entirely from the protein of the food, the carbohydrate and the fat of the diet being useful only to protect a part of this protein from oxidation. Voit's experiments have been shown by Pniiger to have been based upon erroneous analyses of the meat used in his experiments. Voit assumed that in this meat the ratio -f alcohol when oxidized furnishes as much heat as 1 .7 gms. of sugar or 0.75 gm. of fat. But I gm. of alcohol when burnt yields only 1.91 gms. of ('<>.,, while 1.7 gin-, of sugar yield 2.77 gms. CO,, and 0.75 gm. of fat, 2.13 gms. of CO,. If f.-it were replaced by the alcohol the amount of COa should be reduced about L0 per cent., while if the sugar were replaced the reduction should amount i" 31 per rent. Thai such a reduction is not actually observed is ined by the fact thai the alcohol leads to more muscular activity and i | heal from the congested skin, thus indirectly augmenting tin- i) cidatione of I be body. To determine whetherthe combustion of the alcohol protects the protein material from metabolism to the same extent as is done by carbohydrates and fats, experiments have been made in which the individual was broughl into nitrogen equilibrium on a mixed diet. , and Benedict, Bulletin 69, I oited States Department of Agriculture, L889. INORGANIC SALTS, STIMULANTS, AND CONDIMENTS. 889 Then for a given period a portion of the carbohydrate was omitted and alcohol in isodynamic amounts was substituted. The result was an increase in the nitrogen excretion, showing that the alcohol did not protect fully the protein tissue. In a third period the first diet was resumed, and after nitrogen equilibrium had again been established the same proportion of carbohydrate was omitted from the diet, but this time alcohol was not substituted. If the diet was poor in protein it was found that less protein was lost from the body when the alcohol was omitted than when it was used. Hence alcohol not only did not take the place of the carbohydrate in protecting the protein, but it actually caused an increased pro- tein consumption.* Other observers (Neumann, Rosemann f ) have found that, although the effect just described may occur in the first few days, yet if the alcohol diet is maintained the injurious effect exercised by it disappears, the body ceases to lose its protein tissue, and may even lay on protein. These results, taken with those given above, indicate, therefore, that the alcohol may actually take the place physiologically of fat or carbohydrates as a source of energy and as a protector of protein metabolism. J Under these circumstances, therefore, it acts as a true foodstuff. It is perhaps scarcely necessary to emphasize the fact that this scientific con- clusion does not mean that alcohol can be regarded as a prac- tical food. Its expensiveness, its dangers when the dose is too large, etc., prevent us from regarding it in this light. As Rosemann says, however, it is possible that on account of its ready absorption and palatableness it may form a useful substitute for the solid, non-nitrogenous foodstuffs in sickness. This suggestion seems to be supported by many reports of cases in which alcohol has served as the sole or main nutriment during the critical periods of fevers and in other conditions, but it needs to be tested more carefully by direct experiments before it can be accepted generally for prac- tical purposes. In line with this suggestion there are some results upon diabetic patients (Benedict and Torok) which indi- cate that in this condition alcohol used as a food diminishes the production of acetone bodies and protects the protein. '* See Miura, "Zeitschrift fur klin. Medicin," 20, 1892. _ f See Rosemann, "Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 86, 307, 1901, and 100, 348, 1903, for discussion and literature. | See also Atwater and Benedict, "Memoirs of National Academy of Sciences,'' 1902; and Atwater, "The Nutritive Value of Alcohol," in "Physi- ological Aspects of the Liquor Problem," vol. ii., 1903. CHAPTER L. EFFECT OF MUSCULAR WORK AND TEMPERATURE ON BODY METABOLISM— HEAT ENERGY OF FOODS— DIETETICS. The Effect of Muscular Work. — It is a matter of common knowledge that muscular exercise increases the food consumed, and scientific experiments have shown that it greatly augments the consumption of material in the body. Physiologists have attempted to determine which of our energy-yielding foodstuffs is directly affected by muscular activity. A brief statement of the development of our knowledge upon this point will make clear our present theories. According to Liebig, our foods fulfill two general purposes in the body : they are burnt to supply heat, respira- tory foods — fats, and carbohydrates, or they are used to construct tissue, plastic foods — proteins. It seemed to follow, from this generalization, that muscular tissue in activity should use protein material, and it was believed at that time that the metabolism of protein furnished the source of muscular energy. That it is not the sole source was demonstrated by the interesting experiments of lick and Wislicenus. These physiologists ascended the Faul- horn to a height of 1956 meters. Knowing the weight of his body, each could estimate how much work was done in ascending such a height, kick's weight, for example, was 66 kilograms; therefore in climbing the mountain he performed 66X1956=129,096 kilo- grammeters of work, in addition, the work of the heart and the respiratory muscles, which could not be determined accurately, v.,,- estimated at 30,000 kilogrammeters. There was, moreover, a certain amount of muscular work performed in the move- incut- of the arms and in walking upon level ground that was omitted entirely from their calculations. For seventeen hours before the ascent, during the climb of eight hours, and for six hour.- afterward their food was entirely non-nitrogenous, so that the urea eliminated came entirely from the protein of the body. i [i '.hen the urine was collected and the urea estimated, it was found that the energy contained in the protein destroyed, reckoned as heal energy, was entirely insufficient to account for the work done. Although later estimates would modify somewhat the actual figures of their calculation, the margin was so great that 890 EFFECT OF MUSCULAR WORK AND TEMPERATURE. 891 the experiment has been accepted as showing conclusively that the total energy of muscular work does not come necessarily from the oxidation of protein. Later experiments made by Yoit upon a dog working in a tread-wheel and upon a man performing work while in the respiratory chamber gave the surprising result that not only may the energy of muscular work be far greater than the heat energy of the protein simultaneously oxidized, but that the performance of muscular work within certain limits does not affect at all the amount of protein metabolized in the body, since the output of urea is the same on working days as during days of rest. Careful experiments by an English physiologist, Parkes, made upon soldiers, while resting and after performing long marches, showed also that there is no distinct increase in the secretion of urea after muscular exercise. It followed from these latter experiments that Liebig's theory as to the source of the energy of muscular work is incorrect, and that the increase in the oxidations in the body, which undoubtedly occurs during muscular activity, must affect only the non-protein material — that is, the fats and carbohydrates. Subsequently the question was reopened by experiments made under Pfliiger by Argutinsky.* In these experiments the total nitrogen excreted was determined with especial care in the sweat as well as in the urine and the feces. The muscular work done consisted in long walks and mountain climbs. Argutinsky found that work caused a marked increase in the elimination of nitrogen, the increase extending over a period of three days, and he estimated that the additional protein metabolized in consequence of the work was sufficient to account for most of the energy expended in per- forming the walks and climbs. A number of objections have been made to Argutinsky's work. It has been asserted that during his experiment he kept himself upon a diet deficient in non-protein material, and that if the supply of this material had been sufficient there would not have been an increase in protein metabolism. These experiments were repeated in various forms by many ob- servers (Zuntz, Speck, et al.), and the general result has been the abandonment of both the former views — the Liebig theory, that the energy comes only from the consumption of protein, and the Voit theory, that it comes only from the oxidation of non-pro- tein material. It has been found that in muscular work carried to the ordinary extent protein material, in excess of that destroyed in conditions of rest, may or may not be used according to the amount of fats and carbohydrates contained in the diet. If these latter elements are in sufficient quantity they furnish the energy required, and the protein metabolism is not increased by work. If, however. * Argutinsky, "Pfluger's Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 46, 552. 1890. 892 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. the non-proteins are not sufficient in quantity some of the energy- is obtained at the expense of the protein of the body, and there is an increase in the nitrogen excretion. We may believe, in fact, that the energy necessary for muscular work may be obtained from any of the heat-yielding foodstuffs — carbohydrates, fat, or proteins. It seems probable that the sugar (glycogen) of the muscle is, so to speak, the easiest source; but, when the carbohydrates are deficient or absent altogether in the diet, muscular exercise is accompanied by an increase in the consumption of fats or proteins or both. According to the view adopted in the preceding pages, it will lie re- membered that when protein-food is used as a source of energy it is used not as protein, but after the nitrogen has been split off in the liver by the process of deamidization of the ammo-acids. According to this view, therefore, the working muscle cells obtain their energy always by oxidation of non-nitrogenous material, although a portion of this material may have been derived ulti- mately from the protein of the food. The Voit theory is correct to the extent that on an abundant non-protein diet much muscular work may be done without any increase in the consumption of protein tissue. The muscle is a protein machine for the accom- plishment of work, but in the performance of moderate work there is apparently no greater wear and tear of the machinery, no greater tissue waste, than under resting conditions. If, however, the muscular work is excessive, the tissue waste may be increased. Argutinsky found an increased nitrogen elimination lasting two or three days after the cessation of the work. It is probable that this result indicates a greater waste of the protein apparatus itself, and this idea is borne out by the fact that under similar conditions other observers have detected an increase in the creatinin and uric acid excretion, nitrogenous wastes that are derived from muscle. The effect of muscular work on the carbon excretion, car- bon dioxid, is, of course, marked and invariable. Some extra ma- terial iuu-t be oxidized to furnish the energy, and since this material is usually sugar, or sugar and fat, or the non-nitrogenous portion of the protein of the diel . the effeel , so far as the excret ions are con cemed, will be mo I manifesl in the amounl of carbon dioxid -.riven off. Pettenkofer and Voit found thai the carbon dioxid eliminated by a man during a day of work was nearly double that excreted during a day of rest. Along with this rise in the carbon dioxid excretion there is :i corresponding increase in the absorption of oxygen. Metabolism During Sleep. It- has been shown that during sleep there is no marked change in the total nitrogen excreted, and therefore no distinct decrease in the protein metabolism. According to Siven there La a distinct diminution in the secretion EFFECT OF MUSCULAR WORK AND TEMPERATURE. 893 of the endogenous purin nitrogen. On the contrary, the carbon dioxid eliminated and the oxygen absorbed are unquestionably diminished. This latter fact finds its simplest explanation in the supposition that the muscles are less active during sleep. The muscles do less work in the way of contractions, and, in addition, probably suffer a diminution in tonicity, which also affects their total metabolism. Effect of Variations in Temperature. — In warm-blooded animals variations of outside temperature within ordinary limits do not affect the body temperature. An account of the means by which this regulation is effected will be found in the chapter upon Animal Heat. So long as the temperature of the body remains con- stant, it has been found that a fall of outside temperature may increase the oxidation of non-protein material in the body, the in- crease being in a general way proportional to the fall in tempera- ture. That the increased oxidation affects the non-protein con- stituents is shown by the fact that the urea remains unchanged in quantity, other conditions being the same, while the oxygen con- sumption and the carbon dioxid elimination are increased. This effect of temperature upon the body metabolism is due mainly to a reflex stimulation of the motor nerves to the muscles. The tem- perature nerves of the skin are affected by a fall in outside tempera- ture, and bring about reflexly an increased innervation of the muscles of the body. Indeed, it is stated * that unless the lowering of the temperature is sufficient to cause shivering or muscular tension no increase in the excretion of C02 results. This fact suf- fices to explain, therefore, the physiological value of shivering and muscular restlessness when the outside temperature is low. The fact that variations in outside temperature affect only the con- sumption of non-protein material falls in, therefore, with the concep- tion of the nature of the metabolism of muscle in activity, given above. When the means of regulating the body temperature break down from too long an exposure to excessively low or ex- cessively high temperatures, the total body metabolism, protein as well as non-protein, increases with a rise in body temperature and decreases with a fall in temperature. In fevers arising from pathological causes it has been shown that there is an increased excretion of nitrogen as well as of carbon dioxid. Effect of Starvation. — A starving animal must live upon the material present in its body. This material consists of the fat stored up, the circulating and tissue protein, and the glycogen. The latter, which is present in comparatively small quantities, is quickly used, disappearing more or less rapidly according to the extent of muscular movements made. Thereafter the animal lives * Johannson, " Skandinavisches Archiv f. Physiologie," 7, 123, 1897. $94 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. on its own protein and fat, and if the starvation is continued to a fatal termination the body becomes correspondingly emaciated. Examination of the several tissues in animals starved to death has brought out some interesting facts. Yoit took two cats of nearly equal weight, fed them equally for ten days, and then killed one to serve as a standard for comparison and starved the other for thirteen days: the latter animal lost 1017 gms. in weight, and the loss was divided as follows among the different organs: Loss TO Supposed Weight Actual, Loss Each 100 Gms. of Organs Before of Organs of Fresh Organ Starvation. in Cms. (Percentage Loss) Bone 393.4 54.7 13.9 Muscle 140S.4 429.4 30.5 Liver 91.9 49.4 53.7 Kidney 25.1 6.5 25.9 Spleen 8.7 5.8 66.7 Pancreas 6.5 1.1 17.0 Testes 2.5 1.0 40.0 Lungs 15.8 2.8 17.7 Heart 11.5 0.3 2.6 Intestines 118.0 20.9 18.0 Brain and cord 40.7 1.3 3.2 Skin and hair 432.8 89.3 20.6 Fat 275.4 267.2 97.0 Blood 138.5 37.3 27.0 Remainder 136.0 50.0 36.8 According to these results, the greatest absolute loss was in the muscles (429 gms.), while the greatest percentage loss was in the fat (97 per cent.), which had practically disappeared from the body. It is very significant that the central nervous system and the heart, organs which we may suppose were in continual activity, suffered practically no loss of weight: they had lived at the expense of the other tissues. We must suppose that in a starving animal the fat and (he protein materials, particularly in the voluntary muscles, pi - into solution in the blood, and are then used to nourish the tissues generally and to supply the heat necessary to maintain the body temperature. Examination of the excreta in starving ani- mals has shown that a greater quantity of protein is destroyed dur- ing the first day or two than in the subsequent days. This fact plained on the Supposition that the body is at first supplied with a certain excess of protein material derived from its previous food, and that after this is metabolized the animal lives entirely, so far as protein consumption is concerned, upon its "tissue protein." If the animal remains quiet during starvation, the amount of nitrogen excreted daily soon readies a nearly constant minimum, showing that a practically constant amount of protein (together with fat) is consumed daily to furnish body heat, and material for the energy needs and tissue waste in the active organs, Buch as the heart,. Shortly before death from starvation the POTENTIAL ENERGY OF FOOD. 895 daily amount of protein consumed may increase, as shown by the larger amount of nitrogen eliminated. This fact is explained by assuming that the body fat is then exhausted and the animal's metabolism is confined to the tissue proteins alone. The general fact that the loss of protein is greatest during the first one or two days of starvation has been confirmed upon men in a number of interesting experiments made upon professional f asters. For the numerous details as to loss of weight, variations of temperature, etc., carefully recorded in these latter experiments, reference must be made to original sources.* It may be added, in conclusion, that the fatter the body is, to begin with, the longer will starvation be endured, and if water is consumed freely the evil effects of starvation, as well as the disagreeable sensations of hunger, are very much reduced. The Potential Energy of Food. — The food material during digestion and after absorption undergoes numerous chemical changes in the body. Some of these changes are not attended by the liberation of heat to any marked extent. Such is the case, for instance, with the hydrolytic cleavages of the molecule which have been described especially in connection with the digestive processes. As an example of this fact one may take the inversion of the double sugars — one molecule of maltose yields two molecules of dextrose. The heat value of a gram molecule of maltose is 1350.7 calories. The heat value of the dextrose resulting from its inversion is 1347.4 cal., so that the process of hydrolysis liberates only 3.3 cal. or about 0.2 per cent, of the total available energy in the maltose.f Similar hydrolytic cleavages occur doubtless within the tissues, and other changes connected with muscular, nervous, and glandular activity, and the building up and breaking down of the living substance take place constantly as a part of general nutritional metabolism. On the other hand, many of the chemical processes occurring in the body are especially valuable on account of the heat liberated. These reactions, for the most part, at least, are oxidations; they are effected under the influence of oxidizing enzymes or by some other means of activating the oxygen. The various stages in the process are not explained, but we know that oxygen is necessary and that the carbon and the hydrogen contained in the substances acted upon appear eventually in the form of oxidation products — namely, carbon dioxid and water — Liebig designated the fats and carbohydrates as respiratory foods on the hypothesis that their fate in the body *"Virchow's Archiv," vol. 131, supplement, 1893; and Luciani, "Das Hungern," 1890. See also Weber, "Ergebnisse der Physiologie, " vol. i., part l., 1902. fSee Herzog, "Zeit. f. physiol. chem.," 37,383,1903, and Tangl, "Pfluger's Archiv," 115, 1, 1906. 896 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. is to be oxidized and furnish heat. While this view is, in the main, correct, it is evident now that a portion at least of the protein molecule, after the splitting off of the nitrogen, may also undergo oxidation and furnish heat. In Liebig's sense, therefore, the proteins play the part of respiratory or heat-pro- ducing foods as well as acting as tissue formers. On the other hand, fats and carbohydrate material may enter to some extent, together with the protein, into the synthesis of cell material, and thus play the role of a plastic or tissue-forming as well as of a respiratory food. We cannot divide the foodstuffs, therefore, strictly into two such classes, but we may perhaps consider the chemical processes in the body under the two heads mentioned above — namely, the oxidation or energy-producing changes and those due to hydrolytic cleavages, synthesis, etc., which are attended by a small liberation of energy, or, indeed, may be accom- panied by an absorption of energy (synthesis). The great supply of heat energy needed by the body to maintain its temperature comes from the oxidation processes. This classification is employed by some physiologists, and is helpful in emphasizing the fact that many chemical changes occur in the body that are of no importance from the standpoint of heat production, and that the changes that do give rise mainly to heat form, as it were, a special group, which is not connected with the building up or breaking down of the living matter, but furnishes the energy by means of which these latter changes and perhaps other functions, such as muscular work, are made possible. Roughly speaking, an adult man forms in his body and gives off to the surrounding air about 2,400,000 calories of heat per day. By calorie or small calorie (c) is meant the quantity of heat necessary to raise 1 gm. of water 1° in temperature. This great supply of heat is derived from the physiological oxidation of the carbohydrate, fat, and protein material of the food. These same materials may be oxi- dized outside the body by burning them at a high temperature or under a high pressure of oxygen, and the heat that they give off in the process can be measured directly. So far as the fats and carbo- hydrates are concerned, the end-products of the oxidation in the body are the same as in their combustion out of the body, and we may believe, therefore, that the amount of heat produced is the same in both cases. Consequently the heat value of a gram of fat or carbohydrate burnt outside the body is spoken of as its combus- tion equivalent, and it measures the amount of potential energy of these foodstuffs which is available for the production of heat or for the supply of energy in other forms to the working cells. With regard to the protein, the case is somewhat different. Its end-products in the body are carbon dioxid, water, and urea POTENTIAL ENERGY OF FOOD. 897 or some other of the nitrogenous waste products. These nitrog- enous wastes are capable of further oxidation with liberation of heat, so that, as far as they are eliminated, the body loses a possible supply of heat energy, which must be subtracted from the total heat energy that the protein gives upon oxida- tion outside the body, in order to determine the available heat energy yielded within the body. The figures obtained for the heat equivalents of the foodstuffs by burning them outside the body in some form of calorimeter are as follows: 1 gm. of fat yields an aver- age of 9300 calories, or 9.3 large calories (C), 1 gm. of carbohydrate yields an average of 4100 calories (4.1 C). These figures may be taken, therefore, to express the quantity of heat given to the body by the oxidation within its tissues of these elements of our food. A gram of protein when burnt outside of the body yields on the aver- age 5778 calories. The heat value of the urea is estimated as 1 gm. = 2523 calories. If we assume that all the nitrogen of the pro- tein appears as urea and that 1 gm. of protein yields J gm. of urea, then the available heat energy of a gram of protein should be equal to 5778 — 841 (or £ of 2523) = 4937 calories. Later workers, however, have given reasons for believing that this last figure is too high. All of the nitrogen is not eliminated as urea, and, moreover, all of the nitrogenous waste is not excreted in the urine; a distinct pro- portion is given off in the feces. Rubner has calculated the avail- able-heat energy of proteins by direct experiments upon animals. In these experiments the heat value of the protein fed was directly determined by burning a sample in a calorimeter. Then after feed- ing a known amount of the protein the urine and feces were col- lected and their heat value was determined in the same way. The difference between the total heat value of the protein fed and the heat value lost in its excreted products in the feces and urine gave the actual heat energy obtained from the protein by the animal body. Results obtained by this method give an average value for 1 gm. protein of 4100 calories (4.1 C), or, since protein contains an average of 16 per cent, of nitrogen, we may say that 1 gm. of ni- trogen ingested as protein has a heat value of 4.1 X 6.25 = 25.6 C. The figures that are used, therefore, in estimating the heat value of our foodstuffs are: 1 gm. proteid = 4100 calories (4.1 C). 1 gm. carbohydrate = 4100 calories (4.1 C). 1 gm. fat = 9305 calories (9.3 C). Making use of these values, it is obvious that we can calculate the total heat value of any given diet. If we analyze the food for its composition in the three principal foodstuffs we may determine how many calories will be furnished to the body. In many of the tables published to show the composition of the different foods figures are 57 898 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. Fat. Cahbohy- Ash_ Heat Value in Calories DRATE. Per Pound. 17.9 0.8 1110 2.4 1.2 475 18.7 4.0 1280 3.7 1.1 520 5.3 1.3 575 10.3 0.9 740 14.2 1.3 1060 24.2 0.8 1270 12.3 0.7 775 4.8 0.7 380 3.8 2.6 1.5 600 10.7 0.7 680 4.0 5.0 0.7 325 7.2 67.5 1.9 1860 0.3 79.0 0.4 1630 1.9 71.9 1.0 1675 0.5 ] 16.9 1.0 465 0.1 18.4 1.0 385 0.3 3.2 2.1 110 0.4 3.9 0.5 105 0.5 14.2 0.3 290 0.6 22.0 0.8 460 given also to express their heat value or potential energy, on the belief that, for the most part, our food is used as fuel to supply energy to the body. These values for some of our ordinal foods are as follows : * Protein. Beefsteak, porterhouse 19.1 Beefsteak, round (lean) 20.2 Corned beef (canned) 26.3 Veal, les; (lean) 19.4 Veal liver 19.0 Mutton, leg (lean) 16.5 Pork, ham (fresh, lean) 24.8 Pork chops, medium fat 13.4 Chicken (fowl) 13.7 Shad 9.4 Shad roe 20.9 Eggs 11.7 Milk 3.3 Oatmeal 16.1 Rice 8.0 Wheat flour (entire wheat) 13.8 Green peas 7.0 Potatoes (raw) 2.2 Spinach 2.1 Tomatoes 0.9 Apples 0.4 Bananas 1.3 It must be borne in mind, however, that the entire nutritional value of a food is not expressed in its heat value. Some of our# food material — the green foods and fruits, for example — -are useful and in a measure essential because of their salts and organic acids. In a general way, however, the heat energy of a food expresses its value as a means for supplying the energy needs of the living cells. In the work that these cells perform, whether it is contrac- tion, secretion, or nervous activity, energy is needed, and this energy is carried into the body in the potential chemical energy of the proteins, fats, and carbohydrates. Dietetics. — The subject of the proper nourishment of individ- uals or collection of individuals in health and in sickness is treated usually in works upon hygiene or dietetics. The practical details of the preparation and composition of diets must be obtained from such sources. f The general principles upon which practical dieting depends have been obtained, however, from experimental work upon the nutrition of man and the lower animals, some account of which has been given in the foregoing pages. In a healthy adult the main objects of a diet are to furnish sufficient nitrogenous and non-nitrogenous foodstuffs, salts, and water to * Selected from At water and Bryant, Bulletin 28 (revised edition), United States Departmenl of Agriculture, L889. f For practical directions, see Guiltier, " L'aliinentation et les regimes, " I'.ioi: Blyth, "foods: their Composition and Analysis." DIETETICS. 899 maintain the body in an equilibrium of material and of energy — that is, the diet must furnish the material for the regeneration of tissue and the material for the heat produced and the muscular work and other work done. Nutritional experiments prove that this object may be accomplished by protein food alone, together with salts and water. It is doubtful, however, whether, in the case of man, such a diet could be continued for long periods without causing some nutritional disturbance, directly or indirectly. It will be remembered that a pure meat diet is not entirely protein, since all flesh contains some fats and carbohydrates (glycogen). The functions of a diet are accomplished more easily and more economically when it is composed of proteins and fats, or proteins and carbohydrates, or, as is almost universally the case, of proteins, fats, and carbohydrates. The experience of mankind shows that such a mixed diet is most beneficial to the body and most satisfying to that valuable regulating mechanism of nutrition, the appetite. Expressed in its most general form the cells of our body need food for two purposes: first, to supply the energy needs; second, to furnish the material for the construction of their own living substance, that is, for assimilation. The first of these purposes is fulfilled by any of the three energy-yielding foodstuffs, carbohydrates, fats or proteins, but as a matter of fact we use chiefly the carbohydrates on account of their economy and the ease with which they are utilized by the body. For the second purpose, the construction of protoplasm or living matter proteins (or their cleavage products) are absolutely necessary. Whether fats or carbohydrates participate at all in this process is perhaps an open question. In accordance with this specific and necessary function of the protein we find that the amount used in the daily diet is fairly constant, about 100 grams, while the proportions of fat and carbohydrate show wide variations. Since from the energy standpoint the fats and carbohydrates have a common function, serving as fuel for the energy needs of the body, we ought to be able to exchange them in the diet in the ratio of their heat values. This ratio, or as it is frequently called, the isodynamic equiva- lent, is as 9.3 to 4.1 or 2.3 to 1, and within the limits permitted by the appetite we should be able to substitute 1 part of fat for 2.3 parts of sugar or starch. Experiments upon animals, as well as the experience of mankind, sIioav that this substitution can be made in a general way, although it is not advisable to eliminate either of these foodstuffs entirely from the diet. The fact that within certain limits fats and carbohydrates may be substituted for each other is illustrated in a general way by the different diets recommended by various physiologists, since it will be noticed 900 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. that in those in which the proportion of fat is large the amount of carbohydrate is reduced. AVERAGE DIETS AND THEIR HEAT VALUES. MOLESCHOTT. RANKE. VoiT. Calories. Calories. Calories. Protein 130 gms. . . . 533 100 gms. . . .410 118 gms. ... 483 Fats 40 " ... 372 100 " ... 930 56 " ... 520 Carbohydrates . . . 550 " . . . 2275 240 " . . . 984 500 " . . . 2050 2980 2324 3053 FORSTER. . .930 . . 984 56 500 " 2324 Atwater. Calories. 125 gms. 125 " 400 " 512 1172 1640 Calories. Protein 131 gms. . . . 567 Fats 68 " ... 632 Carbohydrates . .494 " . . .1825 "2024 3324 The average heat value of these diets is equal to 2742 calories, of which about 18 per cent, is furnished by the protein. Generally speaking, it will be found that in the dietaries selected voluntarily by mankind the protein furnishes from 15 to 20 per cent, of the total heat value of the diet. According to some physiologists this proportion is unnecessarily large and it might be reduced to as little as 5 or 10 per cent. Whether or not such a change is justified has already been discussed to some extent (p. 858). Leaving aside this point, it is usually estimated in round numbers that the diet should furnish daily 2400 calories for an individual weighing 60 kgms., or about 40 calories per kgm. of body weight. It will be noticed that in all cases the greatest portion of this energy is obtained from the carbohydrate food, which, on account of its economy, its abundance, and its ease of digestion and oxidation in the body, constitutes the bulk of our diet. In cases of excessive muscular work the food eaten may supply more than twice the average heat value given above. Thus, Atwater and ►Sherman estimate that in a six-day bicycle race by professionals the heat value of the food for the different participants varied from 4770 to 6005 calories. Chittenden, in the work previously re- ferred to,* has raised the question whether the heat value of the diet ordinarily employed is unnecessarily high. In his own case he found that the body could be well nourished on a diet con- taining a total heat value of only 1600 calories or 28 calories per kgm. of body weight instead of 40 calories. The diet in this case, it will be remembered, contained only 36 to 40 gms. of protein in place of the 100 to l.'iO gins, recommended in the diets mentioned above. The question thus raised is one that must bo decided by actual experience, but from the numerous statistical and experi- mental results riow availablef it would appear, as has been stated * Chittenden, "Physiological Kconomy in Nutrition," L905. f See especially the numerous Bulletins of the U.S. Department of Agriculture, Nos. 28, I L6, L29, L49, etc. DIETETICS. 901 above, that the total energy necessary in a diet, estimated in terms of its heat value, varies chiefly with the amount of muscular work to be done. Persons who lead a very muscular life require a correspondingly large amount of energy in the diet, and this demand is met usually by augmenting the proportion of carbo- hydrate and fat, especially the carbohydrate. Since the amount of protein is not varied greatly in such cases the diet is relatively poor in this foodstuff. On the contrary, those who lead a sedentary life, including, broadly speaking, all the well-to-do class, require less energy in their diet, and they can afford to reduce the pro- portion of carbohydrate and fat. The diet in such cases may be relatively rich in protein, although the amount per kilogram of body weight is not increased, in fact, is usually diminished some- what. These facts are illustrated in Atwater's estimate of the diet necessary for men performing different amounts of muscular work. Protein. Carbohydrate and Fat. Man doing hard muscular work 600 cal. 3550 cal. Man doing moderate muscular work 500 " 2900 " Man doing no muscular work 360 '; 2040 '; On comparing these diets it will be observed that in per- forming hard muscular work the diet contained 1700 calories of energy beyond that used when no work was done. About six- sevenths of this increase was provided for by the carborrydrates and fats. It will be seen also that in this case the proportion of the total energy obtained from protein remained practically identical. Mankind is guided and has been guided in all times by the control of the appetite, using this term in a general sense to designate the conscious desire for food, and also the desire, more or less clearly recognized, for special kinds of food. If scientific experiments indicate that this regulatory apparatus leads us to ingest more food than is actually required for the assimilation needs and the energy needs of the body, it remains for observa- tion and experiment to determine whether this excess is beneficial or useless or, perhaps, even harmful. Munk gives an interesting table showing how much of certain familiar articles of food would be necessaiy , if taken alone, to supply the requisite daily amount of protein or non-protein material ; his estimates are based upon the percentage composition of the foods and upon experimental data showing the extent of absorption of the foodstuffs in each food. In this table he supposes that the daily diet should contain 110 gms. of protein = 17.5 gms. of N. and non- proteins sufficient to contain 270 gms. of C: 902 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. Milk Meat (lean) . . Hen's eggs... Wheat flour. . Wheat bread. Rye bread . . . Rice Corn Peas Potatoes .... For 110 Gms. Proteih (17.5 Gms. N). 2900 gms. 540 " 18 eggs. 800 gms. . . 1650 " . . 1900 " . . 1870 " 990 " 520 " . . 4500 " For 270 Gms. V. 3800 gms. 2000 " 37 eggs. 670 gms. 1000 " 1100 " 750 " 660 " 750 " 2550 " As Munk points out, this table shows that any single food, if taken in quantities sufficient to supply the nitrogen, would give too much or too little carbon and the reverse; those animal foods which, in certain amounts, supply the nitrogen needed furnish only from one- fourth to two-thirds of the necessary amount of carbon and, vice versa, the vegetable foods if taken in sufficient quantity to supply the carbon would not give sufficient nitrogen, or if used alone to furnish the requisite nitrogen would give an excess of carbon. This same fact is illustrated in another way in a table compiled by Cohnheim.* To furnish the body with its necessary daily quota of 100 grams of protein the following amounts of different foods, expressed in their heat values, would be required: Meat 495 Eggs 1133 Cheese 1704 Milk 2070 Corn 4104 Coarse bread 4552 Fine bread 4720 Potatoes 5000 Rice 5600 It is evident from this table that a person leading a sedentary life who used a vegetable diet alone would be required, in order to obtain his necessary protein, to consume much more carbohy- drate than from an energy standpoint was needed by the body. As ( 'ohnheim points out, the animal foods are for this reason espe- cially suited to supply the protein needs of those who lead a com- paratively inactive life. In practical dieting we are accustomed to get our supply of proteins, fats, and carbohydrates from both vegetable and animal foods. To illustrate this fact by an actual case, in which the food was carefully analyzed, an experimenter weighing 67 k^ms. records that he kept himself in nitrogen equilib- rium upon a diet in which the protein was distributed as follows: N. :;nii •/hi,-, meal = 63.08 gms. protein = 9.78 gms (106.3 <-.c. milk = 18.74 " = 2.905 " loo gms. rice = 7.74 " = 1.2 loo " bread = 11.32 " = 1 .755 " 500 c.c. wine = 1.17 " = 0.182 gin. 102.05 " " = 15.868 gms. " * Cohnheim, "Die Physiologie der Verdauungund Ernahrung," 1908. DIETETICS. 903 For a person in health and leading an active, normal life, appetite and experience seem to be safe and sufficient guides by which to control the diet; they may be relied upon, at least, to protect the body from undernutrition. The opposite danger of overeating is a real one, particularly among those who do not lead an active life. It is, however, a hygienic offence that is usually committed knowingly and may consequently be controlled by those who have sufficient wisdom. Physiological knowledge emphasizes clearly enough the great fact that the mechanisms of nutrition and digestion, like the other mechanisms of the body, should not be subjected to unnecessary strain. For those who are in health, the important rule to follow in the matter of diet is to avoid an excess in eating. In conditions of disease, in regulating the diet of children or of collections of individuals, as in the army, navy, etc., it is necessary for purposes of hygiene or for purposes of economy to arrange the diet in accordance with the knowledge obtained from experience and from scientific investigations. In this direction much has already been accomplished, but more remains to be done, particularly perhaps in the relation of diet to pathological conditions. CHAPTER LI. THE PRODUCTION OF HEAT IN THE BODY— ITS MEAS- UREMENT AND REGULATION— BODY TEMPERA- TURE—CALORIMETRY— PHYSIOLOGICAL OXIDATIONS. It is customary to date our modern ideas of the origin of animal heat from the time of Lavoisier (1774-77). To the older physiologists it was a most difficult problem. The animal's body produces heat continually and maintains a temperature higher, as a rule, than that of the surrounding air. Since oxygen and the nature of ordinary combustions were unknown, they naturally explained this heat formation by reference to causes which the science of the day had shown to be capable of producing warmth, such as friction and fermentation. Haller (1757), for instance, taught that the body heat arises mainly from the friction of the circulating blood and the movements of the heart and blood-vessels, and this view found currency in text-books well into the nine- teenth century. Lavoisier first gave to the physiologist the con- ception that the heat produced in the body is due to a combustion or oxidation, and that therein lies the significance of our respiration of oxygen. He believed himself that this oxidation takes place in the lungs, — that is, the blood brings to the lungs a hydrocarbon- ous material which is attacked by the oxygen and burnt with the formation of water and carbon dioxid and the liberation of heat. Later experimenters demonstrated that the heat production does not occur in the lungs, at least not exclusively, but over the whole of the body. After a long and interesting controversy it was also shown satisfactorily that the oxidations of the body do not occur in the blood, but in the tissues themselves. The oxygen is transported to the cells and there does its work of effecting oxi- dations and giving rise to heat. This heat is equalized more or [ess over the whole body, chiefly by the circulation of the blood, which absorbs heat from the warmer organs and distributes it to the cooler ones. The body temperature is maintained at a nearly constant level by an intricate adjustment of physiological reflexes which together constitute the heat-regulating mechanism. Such in brief is the general theory of our time regarding heat production in the body. Many of the problems that interested the older phys- 904 BODY TEMPERATURE. 905 iologists have been solved satisfactorily, but there remain, of course, many more to interest this and succeeding generations. Investi- gations in this field at present are directed mainly to an effort to understand the details of the heat-regulating apparatus, on the one hand, and, on the other, to comprehend more satisfactorily the nature of the process of oxidation. This latter problem is one of common interest at present in chemistry and in physiology. The Body Temperature. — We divide animals into the "two great classes of warm blooded and cold blooded, according as their temperature is or is not above that of the surrounding air. In this sense, birds and mammals are warm blooded and reptiles, amphibia, and fishes are cold blooded. The names, however, are badly chosen. The difference of deepest significance between the mammals and birds, on the one hand, and the fishes, amphibia, and reptiles, on the other, is that in the former the body temperature is, within wide limits, independent of the outside temperature ; it remains practically constant during winter and summer, whether the surrounding air is hotter or cooler than the body. They are, therefore, constant-temperature animals (homoiothermous). The reptiles, amphibia, and fishes, on the contrary, have a body tem- perature that changes with the environment. On winter da}rs their temperature is low, approximately that of the surrounding air or water, and in summer their body temperature rises to cor- respond with that of the outside. Strictly speaking, they are cold blooded only in cold surroundings. This group may be designated as the changeable-temperature animals (poikilothermous). The warm-blooded animals maintain a constant high body temperature on account of their relatively active oxidations and the existence of a heat -regulating mechanism. In the cold-blooded animals the oxidations are not so intense and a heat-regulating mechanism is absent or poorly developed. The hibernating animals form a group intermediate in many ways between these two classes. They possess a heat-regulating apparatus that maintains a constant body tem- perature under most conditions, but breaks down in very cold weather; so that during the period of winter sleep their tem- perature is but little above that of the surrounding air. In some of the cold-blooded animals the production of heat is more rapid during warm weather than its loss; so that they exhibit a body temperature slightly higher than the surrounding me- dium. A hive of bees in activity may raise the temperature within the hive through a number of degrees and snakes and many reptiles show a temperature of 2° to 8° C. above that of the air. So also some reptiles possess a rudimentary means of protecting their bodies from too great a rise of temperature, — for instance, by accelerated breathing whereby more water is evap- 906 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. orated from the lungs and thus more heat is lost.* The distinc- tion between the two great groups of animals is not entirely abso- lute, but it is sufficiently marked to constitute a striking physio- logical characteristic. The temperature of the human body is measured usually by thermometers placed in the mouth, in the axilla, or in the rectum. Measurements made in this way show that in general the tempera- ture in the interior of the bodjr (rectal) is slightly higher than on the surface of the skin. The average temperature in the rectum is 37.2° C. (9S.960 F.); in the axilla, 36.9° C. (98.45° F.); in the mouth, 36.87° C. (98.36° F.). We may speak of the body temperature, therefore, in the places in which it can be conveniently measured, as varying between 36.87° C. and 37.2° C. Some of the internal or- gans have a higher temperature, particularly during their period of greatest activity. The temperature of man, measured in the places mentioned, shows also a distinct variation during the day, a diurnal rhythm. This daily variation has been measured by many ob- servers, and shows individual peculiarities that depend largely upon the manner of living, time of meals, etc. In general it may be said that the lowest temperature is shown early in the morning, — 6 to 7 a.m.; that it rises slowly during the day to reach its maximum in the evening, 5 to 7 p.m. ; and falls again during the night. The difference between early morning and late afternoon or evening may amount to a degree or more centigrade, and this fact must be borne in mind by physicians when observing the temperature of patients. Muscular activity and food appear to be the factors that are mainly responsible for the rise in temperature during the day. Most observers state that when the habits of life are reversed for some time — that is, when work is performed and meals are eaten during the night, and the day is given up to sleep and rest — the daily variation of temperature is inverted to correspond, — that is, the highest temperature is observed in the early morning and the lowest in the late afternoon. Age also has a slight influence. Newly born infants and young children have a somewhat higher temperature than adults. The difference may amount to half a degree or a degree centigrade, — 37.6° C. in infants as compared with 36.6° C. or 37. 1° C. in the adult. It is known, also, that the heat- regulating mechanism in infants and young children is not so efficient as in adults, and that therefore febrile disturbances are more easily excited in the former than in the latter. In the matter of body temperature, as in so many other characteristics, aged people show a tendency to revert to infantile conditions. Their temperature, according to most observers, is slightly higher than in middle life. * See Langlois, "Journal de physiologie et de pathol. gOnOrale," 1902, 249. CALORIMETRY. 907 Among physiological conditions that influence the body tempera- ture, muscular work and meals, as stated above, have the most posi- tive effect. Marked muscular activity implies a great increase in the production of heat in the body and most observers find that the initial result at least is a small rise in body temperature, — a fact which indicates that the heat regulation is not perfect; the excess of heat produced is not dissipated promptly. In the period of rest following upon work, on the contrary, the body temperature may fall, owing probably to the fact that more heat is lost through the flushed skin than is produced within the body. In this matter of the effect of muscular work individual variations are to be expected, since the perfection of the heat-regulating mechanisms may vary somewhat in different persons. Meals also cause a slight rise in body temperature, which reaches its maximum about an hour and a half after the ingestion of the food. The explanation in this case also is to be found doubtless in a greater production of heat, due to the increased metabolism in the secreting glands, the liver, and the musculature of the gastro-intestinal canal. The excessive production of heat is not compensated completely by a corresponding increase in the heat dissipated.* It is sufficiently obvious, perhaps, from these facts that the temperature as measured by the thermometer is a balance between the amount of heat produced and the amount of heat lost or dissipated. The thermometer alone gives us no cer- tain indication of the quanity of heat produced in the body. A temperature higher than normal, fever temperature, may be due either to an excessive production of heat or to a deficient dissipa- tion. To understand and control the processes by which the body temperature is kept normal it is necessary to discover a means for ascertaining at any time the actual quantities of heat produced and dissipated, and the effect upon each factor of different normal and pathological conditions. The method used for determining the quantity of heat is designated as calorimetry. It is necessary, therefore, to describe the principle and construction of calorimeters and the methods of calorimetry before attempting to explain the mechanism of heat regulation. Calorimetry. — A calorimeter is an instrument for measuring the quantity of heat given off from a body. The unit employed in these determinations is the calorie, — that is, the amount of heat necessary to raise 1 gm. of water 1° C.,or more accurately the amount of heat required to raise 1 gm. of water from 15° to 16° C. This unit is sometimes designated as a small calorie to distinguish it from the large calorie (C), — that is, the quantity of heat necessary to raise 1 kgm. of water 1° C. The large calorie is equal to 1000 * For further details see Puchet, "La chaleur animate," 1889; and Pem- brey, " Animal Heat, " Schaefer's " Text-book of Physiology, " vol. i, 1898. 908 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. small calories. In physiology calorimeters have been used for two main purposes: to determine the heat equivalent of foods, — that is, the amount of heat given off when the various foodstuffs are burned, — and, secondly, to determine the heat produced and the heat dissi- pated by living animals during a given period. For the first pur- pose the apparatus that is most frequently employed at present is the bomb calorimeter devised by Berthelot. The bomb consists of a strong steel cylinder in which the food to be burned is placed CT\ EWT Fig. 290. — Reichert's water calorimeter. and which is filled with oxygen. The combustion of the foodstuff is initiated by means of a spiral of platinum wire heated by an electrical current. The bomb is immersed in water and the heat given off raises the water to a measured extent of temperature. The weight of water being known, the amount of heat is easily expressed in calories. For the purpose of measuring the heat given off by living animals two principal forms of calorimeter are used, each form having a number of modifications. These two forms are the water calorimeter and the air calorimeter. The water calorimeter was the form used in the first experiments on rec- ord (Crawford, 1779). In principle it consists of a double-walled box with a known weight of water between the walls. The animal is placed in the inner box and the heat given off is absorbed by the CALORIMETRY. 909 water. Knowing the weight of the water and how much its tem- perature is raised, the data are at hand for determining the number of calories given off during the experiment. One form of this variety of calorimeter, used in this country by Reichert, is shown in Fig. 290. It consists of two concentric boxes of metal with a space between them of about 1-y inches. The animal is placed in the inner box (A). The two boxes are inclosed in a large wooden box, the space between the metal and wooden boxes being filled with shavings (SH). The object of this outer box is to prevent radiation of heat from the metal boxes. The tubes EN and EX, which lead into the interior chamber containing the animal, are for the entrance and exit of the ventilating air. A thermometer is placed in each to determine the heat carried off by the air. The thermometer, CT, measures the temperature of the water, and S is a stirrer to keep the water well mixed and thus insure a uniform temperature. When the animal is placed in the apparatus the heat given off warms not only the water, but also the metal; so that to determine the total heat the weight of metal must be re- duced to an equivalent amount of water by multiplying its weight by its specific heat, or, a more simple method, the calorimetric equiv- alent of the apparatus is determined, — that is, the actual amount of heat necessary to raise the temperature of the apparatus, water and metal, one degree. This value is obtained by burning in the appa- ratus a known weight of some substance (alcohol, hydrogen) whose heat of combustion is known. Knowing how much heat is given off by this combustion and how much the temperature of the apparatus is raised, the calorimetric equivalent is easily calcu- lated and may be used subsequently in estimating the results ob- tained from animals. In the use of the apparatus many precau- tions must be observed. These practical details need not be des- cribed here except to say that account must be taken of the warm- ing of the air used to ventilate the apparatus and of any changes in the amount of its moisture. The calorimeter used in this way measures directly the amount of heat given off from the animal during the period of observation. The amount of heat produced in the animal's body during this time may be the same, or may be more or less. To arrive at a knowledge of this factor observations must be made upon the animal's body temperature by means of a thermometer in the rectum. If this body temperature is the same at the end as at the beginning of the experiment then it is obvious that the heat produced must have been equal to the heat lost. If the animal's body temperature has fallen, then it is evident that less heat has been produced than was lost. To ascertain how much less, the weight of the animal is multiplied by its specific heat (0.8) to reduce it to so much water, and this product is multiplied by the difference in body temperature at the beginning and the end of the 910 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. experiment. The product is obtained in calories and is subtracted from the amount of heat lost, as determined by the calorimeter, to obtain the amount of heat produced. If, on the contrary, the ani- mal's temperature has risen during the experiment the body has produced more heat than it has dissipated. The increase may be determined as above by multiplying the weight of the animal, the specific heat of the body, and the difference in temperature. This amount added to the heat lost gives the heat produced. Many investigators have used some form of air calorimeter. An air calorimeter consists essentially of a double-walled chamber or box with air between the walls. The animal is placed in the inner box and the heat given off is measured by the expansion of the air between the walls. Many different forms are used, prefer- Fig. 291. — D'Arsonval's differential calorimeter. ence being given to some modification of the differential air calo- rimeter. In this last-named instrument two exactly similar chambers are constructed ; one contains the animal while the other serves as a dummy. These two chambers are balanced against each other, the air space in the dummy being heated by immersion in a bath or by burning hydrogen in the interior. As these sources of heat are known and can be controlled, it is evident that if the dummy is made to balance exactly the chamber containing the animal the amount of heat given off in each is the same. The principle of the differential calorimeter is represented in Fig. 291, which gives a schema of the form originally employed by d'Arsonval; 8 and 8' represent the two calorimeters, in one of which the animal is placed while the other acts as dummy. Each is double walled and the air spaces are connected by tubes, 10 and 10', to small gasometers, 4, 4', suspended in water and hung on opposite sides of a balance. The movements of these gasometers antagonize each other, and the resultant may be recorded upon smoked paper, us indicated in the figure.* * For detailed accounts of special forms of air calorimeters see Rubner, ■ Calorimetrische Mefchodik," L891; and Rosenthal, "Arclriv f. Physiologie," 1897, p. 170. CALORIMETRY. 911 The Respiration Calorimeter. — When a calorimeter is so arranged that the composition of the air drawn through the apparatus for ventilation can be determined as well as the amount of heat pro- duced, the apparatus becomes a respiration calorimeter. In such an apparatus, if proper provision is made for analyzing the urine, the feces, and the food, the total carbon and nitrogen excretion may be obtained simultaneously with the heat loss. Since we may calculate from the carbon and nitrogen excretion how much pro- tein, fat, and carbohydrate have been burnt in the body, and since the heat values of these constituents are known, it is evident that we may reckon indirectly how much heat ought to be produced from the combustion of so much material. This method of arriv- ing at the heat production is designated indirect calorimetry . With an adequate respiration calorimeter it is possible to ascertain whether the results calculated by the method of indirect calorim- etry really correspond with the heat obtained by direct measure- ment. In the hands of good observers the correspondence is very close, and gives substantial proof of the scientific belief that in the living body the energy liberated as heat or as heat and work is all contained in potential form in the foodstuffs eaten. By means of the respiration calorimeter we can obtain a balance between the energy income and outgo of the body as well as between the material income and outgo, — that is, the carbon and nitrogen equilibrium. The most complete and elaborate form of respiration calorimeter used is that devised by Atwater and Rosa for experiments upon man.* Considered as a calorimeter the appara- tus used by these investigators belongs to the type of water cal- orimeters. Instead, however, of having a stationary stratum of water to be warmed by the heat given off from the body, the appara- tus is arranged so that a stream of water may be circulated between the walls, and this stream is so regulated, as to quantity and temperature, as to keep the temperature of the calorimeter as a constant point. In other words, the heat given off from the body is carried away by the circulating water, and the quantity of the heat may be calculated when the temperature and amount of the water are known. By means of this apparatus many interesting and important experiments have been made upon the nutrition of man under special conditions. Such results as the following have been obtained (Atwater and Benedict) in the case of a man who, while in the apparatus, did much muscular work on a bicycle ergometer: *See Atwater and Rosa, Bulletin 63, United States Department of Agri- culture, 1899; and for recent improvements, Atwater and Benedict, "A Respira- tion Calorimeter," Carnegie Institution, Washington, 1905. 912 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. Income: Potential energy of material metabolized in body = 5459 Cal. Outor> i Energy given off from the bod}* as heat. . . . 4833 Cal. ° \ Heat equivalent of muscular work 602 Cal. 5435 Cal. 5435 Cal. Experimental error 24 Cal. Results of Calorimetric Measurements. — The actual results obtained from direct calorimetric measurements corroborate those deduced from the study of the energy given off in the oxidation of the foodstuffs of the daily diet. They show that man gives off heat from his body to the amount of 40 to 50 calories per kgm. of weight during 24 hours under conditions of ordinary life, — a total, therefore, of 2400 to 3000 calories per day for an individual weighing 60 kgms. This amount is increased greatly under conditions demanding much muscular work. This loss of heat is, of course, made good by the production of an equal amount within the body by the oxidation of the food material. Actual experiments upon different animals* show that small animals produce more heat in proportion to their weight than larger animals of the same species, owing to their relatively larger surface and, therefore, greater loss of heat. This fact has been expressed by Rubner in what he calls his "surface area law." According to this law the metabolism is proportional to the surface area, or for the same amount of surface area there will be the same pro- duction of heat. He estimates that in man there is produced in 24 hours for each square meter of surface 1042 cal. The figures for other mammals are nearly the same. In small animals of a given species in which the surface area is greater relatively to the mass than in the larger animals, the metabolism per kilogram of weight will be larger — for example, in the human infant com- pared with the adult. HEAT REGULATION. From a general standpoint the most important problem that the physiologist has to study is the means by which the heat production and heat loss are so regulated as to maintain a practically constant body temperature. Experiments show that the mechanism of heat regulation is very complex and is two-sided, — that is, the body possesses means of controlling the loss of heat as well as the produc- tion of heat, and under the conditions of normal life both means are used. Regulation of the Heat Loss.— Heat is regularly lost from our bodies in a number of different ways, which may be classified as follows : *See Rubner, " Zeitschrift. f. Biologie," 19, 535, 1883; and Gesetze, "des Energieverbrauchs," 1002. REGULATION OF HEAT LOSS. 913 1. Through the excreta, urine, feces, saliva, which are at the temperature of the body when voided. 2. Through the expired air. This air is warmer than the inspired ah", and, moreover, is nearly saturated with water-vapor. The vaporiza- tion of water requires heat, which is, of course, taken from the body supply. Each gram of water requires for its vaporization about 0.5 cal. 3. By evaporation of the sweat from the skin. The amount lost in this way increases naturally with the amount of sweat secreted. 4. By conduction and especially by radiation of heat from the skin. The relative values of these different means of heat loss are estimated as follows by Vierordt: 1. By urine and feces 1.8 per cent, or 48 calories. 2. By expired air: Warming of air 3.5 " " 84 Vaporization of water from lungs 7.2 " " 182 3. By evaporation from skin 14.5 " " 364 4. By radiation and conduction from skin 73.0 " " 1792 Total daily loss =2470 It is obvious that the relative importance of these factors will vary with conditions. Thus, high external temperatures will tend to diminish the loss from radiation while increasing that from evapora- tion, owing to the greater production of sweat. The variation in this respect is well illustrated by the following table, compiled by Rubner, from experiments made upon a starving dog:* Temperature. Calories lost by radia- Calories lost by Total calories of tion and conduction. evaporation. metabolism. 7° C. 78.5 7.9 86.4 15° 55.3 7.7 63.0 20° 45.3 10.6 55.9 25° 41.0 13.2 54.2 30° 33.2 23.0 56.2 It will be noted that between 25° and 30° C. there was a marked increase in the loss of heat through evaporation. In man loss of heat is regulated chiefly by controlling the impor- tant factors of evaporation and radiation. We accomplish this end in part deliberately or voluntarily by the use of appropriate clothing. Clothing of any kind captures a layer of warm and moist air between it and the skin and thus diminishes greatly the loss by evaporation and by radiation. In cold weather the amount and character of the clothing is changed in order to diminish the heat loss. The ideal clothing for this purpose is made of material, such as wool, which, while porous enough to permit adequate ventilation of the air next to the skin, is at the same time a poor conductor of heat and thus diminishes the main factor of loss by radiation. The most impor- tant means of controlling the heat loss, however, is by automatic * Taken from Lusk, "Elements of the Science of Nutrition," Philadel- phia, 1906. 58 914 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. reflex control through the sweat nerves and the vasomotor nerves. By these means the amount of perspiration evaporated from the skin and the amount of warm blood sent through the skin are controlled. Rubner speaks of this side of the heat regulation as the phi/sical regulation. By its means the body may be safe- guarded from an abnormal rise of temperature. In warm weather the secretion of sweat is greatly increased by reflex stimulation of the sweat nerves. The greater amount of water requires a greater amount of heat to vaporize it, and thus the heat loss is increased. The value of this control is illustrated by a case recorded by Zuntz* of a man who possessed no sweat glands. In summer this individual was incapacitated for work/ since even a small degree of muscular activity would cause an increase in his body temperature to 40° or 41° C. The control through the vasomotor nerves is doubtless even more important. The blood-vessels bring the warm blood to the skin, where it loses its heat by conduction and especially by radia- tion to the cooler air. When the surrounding air is much below the temperature of the body the vasoconstrictor center is stimulated, the blood-vessels in the skin are constricted, the supply of warm blood to the skin is diminished, and therefore the amount of heat lost is less. The reflex in this case may be attributed primarily to the action of the cool air on the cold nerves of the skin. The impulses carried by these fibers to the nerve centers stimulate the vasocon- strictor center or that part of it controlling the vasomotor fibers to the skin. On warm days, on the contrary, the blood-vessels in the skin are dilated sometimes to an extreme extent, the supply of warm blood is therefore increased, and more heat is lost if the air is lower in temperature than the blood. The reflex in this case may be regarded possibly as an inhibition of the vasoconstrictor center through the warm nerves of the skin. Sub- stances, such as alcohol, which cause a dilatation of the skin ves- sels also increase the loss of body heat, in some cases to a sufficient extent to lower the body temperature. To a smaller extent our heat loss is controlled through an acceleration of the breathing movements. The greatly increased respirations in muscular ac- tivity must aid somewhat in eliminating the excess of heat produced, although this factor must be much less important than the sweating and the flushing of the skin which are produced reflexly during muscular work. In some of the lower animals — the dog, for in- stance— in which the sweat nerves are absent over most of the body and in which the coat of hair interferes with the free loss by radiation, it is found that the loss through the respiratory channel is * Zuntz, " Deutsche medizinal-Zeitung," 1903, No. 25. REGULATION OF HEAT PRODUCTION. 915 relatively more important. The panting of the dog is a familiar phenomenon. Richet has studied this reflex upon dogs and has designated the greatly accelerated breathing in warm weather or after muscular exercise as thermic polypnea (according to Gad, tachypnea). He assumes a special center for the reflex situated in the medulla and acting through the respiratory center. It is a curious fact, as shown by Langlois, that some reptiles exhibit a similar reflex; when their body temperature is raised to 39° C. they show a condition of marked polypnea (rapid breathing) the ap- parent object of which is to augment the loss of heat from the body. Regulation of Heat Production. — Heat production is varied in the body by increasing or decreasing the physiological oxida- tions. This end is effected in part voluntarily by muscular exercise or by taking more food. Muscular contractions are attended by a marked liberation of heat and it is a part of everyone's experience that by work or muscular activity the effect of outside cold may be counteracted. In the case of food the body burns promptly most of the material of a daily diet. By increasing the diet in cold weather provision is made for replacing the greater amount of heat lost from the body without calling upon the tissues of the body itself. In normal individuals this regulation is not, strictly speaking, voluntary. Outside cold is most effective in stimulating the appetite and thus leading us to increase the diet. In this, as in other respects, the appetite serves to control the amount of food in proportion to the needs of the body. The purely involuntary con- trol of heat production consists of an involuntary reflex upon mus- cular metabolism and possibly in the existence of a special set of heat centers and heat nerves. With regard to the first effect we have the striking experiments quoted by Pfliiger,* according to which a rabbit paralyzed by large doses of curare is no longer able to maintain its body temperature when the outside temperature is changed. The rabbit behaves, in fact, like a cold-blooded animal. In the calorimeter it shows a marked loss of heat production, and its temperature may be made to go up and down with the outside temperature. The same result may be obtained by section of all the motor nerves, — that is, section of the spinal cord in the upper cervical region. Rubner has shown by calorimetric experiments upon animals that although the body temperature, as we know, may remain constant when the outside temperature is changed, the heat production is increased as the outside temperature is lowered. This fact is well shown by the following table, compiled by Rubner, from experiments made upon a fasting guinea-pig :f * Pfliiger " Archiv f. die gesammte Physiologie, " 18, 255, 1878. f Taken from Lusk, loc. cit. 916 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. Temperature Temperature Grams of CO^ eliminated per hour of air. of animal. and per kilogram of animal. 0.0° c. 37.0° C. 2.905 11.1 37.2 2.151 20.8 37.4 1.766 25.7 37.0 1.540 30.3 37.7 1.317 34.9 38.2 1.273 40.0 39.5 1.454 From 0° to about 35° C. the animal's body temperature remained practically constant, but the oxidations at the lower temperature were over twice the amount of those at the higher temperature. At about 33° C. the metabolism of the mammal, according to Rubner, is at its minimum. From 35° to 40° C. the heat regula- ting mechanism in the experiments quoted broke down, in that heat loss was prevented to such an extent by the outside high temperature that the body temperature rose in spite of the diminution in heat production. The increased production of heat in the body in consequence of a fall in external temperature is a characteristic property of warm-blooded animals. Rubner designates this side of the regulating mechanism as the chemical regulation, and he calls attention, moreover, to the fact that in mankind, owing to our custom of protecting the surface of the body by clothing and by artificial heat, chemical regulation plays less of a role than in the lower animals. Man, in fact, keeps most of his skin surrounded by a warm layer of air at about the temperature (33° C.) at which the metabolism, as affected by temperature, is minimal. Cold baths, cold winds, and various climatic conditions, such as high altitudes and sea- side conditions, may cause a marked increase in body metabolism. Johannson* has shown that the increased oxidations that occur under the influence of outside cold, as measured by the C02 out- put, occur only when muscular tension is increased or shivering is noticed. We may believe, therefore, that the increased oxida- tions caused by cold are due to motor reflexes upon the skeletal muscles. These reflexes take place doubtless through the motor filicvs. and lead to an augmented muscular tone or to small con- tractions (shivering), according to their intensity. This fact accords with one's personal sensations regarding the condition of his muscles in cold weather. The Existence of Heat Centers and Heat Nerves. — Physi- ologists have long supposed that there may be in the body a special set of heat nerves and heat centers, separate in their action from the motor, secretory, and other efferent nerves that influence the me- * Johannson, " Skandinavisches Arcbiv. f. Physiologic," 7, 123, 1897. EXISTENCE OF HEAT CENTERS AND HEAT NERVES. 917 tabolism of the peripheral organs. It is supposed that these fibers, if they exist, when in activity augment or inhibit the physiological oxidations in the tissues, and that this effect has for its specific object an increase or decrease in heat production, outside of any functional activity of the tissues. Bernard thought at first that he had demonstrated the existence of calorific fibers in the cervical sympathetic, but it was afterward recognized that the fibers in question are vasoconstrictors. Since that time very numerous experiments have been made with this object in view, but it must be admitted that no conclusive proof has yet been obtained of the existence of such a system. The evidence that has been most re- lied upon is the effect of lesions, experimental or pathological, of definite portions of the brain or cord. The following facts are significant: A number of observers* have found that section or puncture of the brain at the junction of medulla and pons causes an increase in heat production and a rise of temperature. Section of the cord in the cervical region is, on the other hand, attended usually by a fall in body temperature. These experiments might be interpreted to mean that there exists in the brain anterior to the medulla a general heat center of an inhibitory character. Under normal conditions this center may hold the lower heat-producing centers in check. When cut off by section this inhibitory influence is removed and increase in heat production and body temperature results. A second important fact, brought out by Ott,f is that in- jury to the corpus striatum causes a rise in heat production and body temperature. This result has been confirmed by many other investigators, making use especially of what is known as the " heat puncture." In this experiment, made upon rabbits, a probe or style is inserted into the brain so as to puncture the corpus stria- tum. The result in the majority of cases is a rise of temperature which may last for a long time, although the animal shows no par- alysis and apparently no other effect from the operation. Accord- ing to some observers,^ the increased production of heat takes place mainly in the liver, and is due to the oxidation of the glycogen. According to others (Aronsohn), the increased production of heat occurs mainly in the muscles. The fever produced by the "heat puncture" seems to be due essentially to an irritation of the nerv- ous system, and is an experimental demonstration of the possi- bility of fever arising from lesions of the nerve centers. White and others have described similar disturbances of heat production from lesions of the optic thalamus. Heat centers have been located * See Wood, "Fever," "Smithsonian Contributions to Knowledge," Washington," 1880. t Ott, "Journal of Nervous and Mental Diseases," 1884, 1887, 1888; also "Brain," 1889. t Roily, "Deutches Archiv f. klinische Medicin," 78, 250, 1903. 918 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. also in the septum lucidum, in the cortex, the midbrain, pons, and medulla, while Reichert places the primary heat-producing centers (thermogenic centers), from which the hypothetical heat nerves originate directly, in the spinal cord in the anterior horn of the gray matter.* The great amount of experimental work done along these lines has been inspired doubtless by the hope of discovering a special heat-regulating nervous apparatus which if demonstrated would enable us to explain the causation of fevers. In its most elaborate form this hypothesis assumes the existence of primary heat-producing (thermogenic) centers in the cord and brain from which the calorific or heat nerves arise. These centers in turn are controlled by regulating (thermotaxic) centers of an augmenting and inhibitory character in the higher portions of the brain. By reflex influences upon these latter centers the activity of the thermo- genic centers may be increased or diminished and the production of heat in the body controlled. While such an apparatus may exist, it is nevertheless true that the evidence in favor of it so far produced has failed to convince the majority of physiologists. The existence of a special set of heat nerves, in fact, is still unproved. Most physiologists, perhaps, believe that variations in heat pro- duction occur, as stated above, by alterations in the intensity of the oxidations in the muscles brought about by reflex excitation through the motor nerve fibers, and that a special set of heat fibers does not exist. We may at present adopt the conservative view that heat production and heat dissipation in the body are controlled not by a special heat-regulating apparatus composed of heat centers and heat nerves, but by the co-ordinated activity of a number of different centers in addition to the voluntary means already specified. The unconscious regulation of the body temperature is effected chiefly through the following centers : !1. The sweat centers and sweat nerves. 2. The vasoconstrictor center and the vasoconstrictor nerve fibers to the skin. 3. The respiratory center. ( 1. The motor nerve centers and the motor nerve fibers tt , i, *• J to the skeletal muscles. neat production s 2 The quantity and character of the food as deter- v. mined by the appetite. Theories of Physiological Oxidations. — Lavoisier compared the oxidations in the body to the oxidation of organic substances in combustions at high temperatures. He supposed that the mo- lecular oxygen unites directly with the substances oxidized in one case as in the other. It soon became evident, however, that this direct analogy is not applicable. The material that is oxidized * See Reichert, " University Medical Magazine," 5, 406, 1894; also Kemp, "Therapeutical Gazette," L889, pp. 66 and 155. PHYSIOLOGICAL OXIDATIONS. 919 in the body — fats, carbohydrates, proteins — is consumed with a certain rapidity, — in the case of muscular contractions with great rapidity, — and we know that these same materials out of the body at a temperature of 39° C. are oxidized with extreme slowness. It became customary, therefore, to speak of the oxidations in the body as indirect, meaning thereby that the material is not acted upon directly by the molecular oxygen . Within recent years it has been shown that the oxidation in ordinary combustions — the burning of gaseous hydrogen, for instance — is not explained by assuming that the oxygen unites directly with the hydrogen. It is stated, for instance, that this combustion does not take place if both gases are entirely free from water vapor; the presence of water is necessary for the oxidation. Chemists are not agreed as to the exact nature of simple combustion, and it is therefore increasingly difficult to compare these processes with the oxidations in the body. Leaving aside the details of the process, it may still be believed that tho metabolism of material in the body by means of which its heat energy is produced is at bottom comparable to ordinary combus- tions. Oxygen is absolutely necessary to the process in each case; the same end-products are formed and the same amount of heat is liberated in the one case as in the other. The fundamental point that the physiologist is attempting to solve is the means by which the body accomplishes these oxidations at such a low temperature. The theories suggested to explain this fact have changed naturally with the advance of chemical knowledge. After the discovery of ozone (Schonbein, 1840) and its great power of oxidation as com- pared with oxygen it was suggested that in some way the oxygen in the body is ozonized and is thus able to burn the food material. Gorup-Besanez showed that some of the oxidations that take place in the body can be successfully accomplished outside the body with the aid of ozone, especially in the presence of alkalies or alka- line carbonates. Others suggested that the oxygen in the body be- comes converted to atomic oxygen and is thus enabled to attack the tissue materials. Hoppe-Seyler formulated a theory according to which the living molecule is first split into smaller molecules by the hydrolytic action of ferments. In this process, as in fermentation, to which he compared it, hydrogen is liberated in the nascent or atomic state, and this hydrogen acting upon the oxygen forms water with the liberation of some atomic oxygen, which in turn oxidizes the split products of the fermentation. Others still (Traube) laid stress upon the possibility of the formation of hydrogen peroxid or similar organic peroxids which are then capable of effecting the oxidation of the body material. This latter theory, in modified form still prevails.* * See Engler and Weissberg, "Kritische Studien uber die Vorgange der Autoxydation, " 1904. 920 NUTRITION" AND HEAT REGULATION. The great amount of experimental and theoretical work upon the nature and cause of physiological oxidations has established pretty clearly two general beliefs which it is important to keep in mind. It has been shown, in the first place, that the amount of the oxidation is governed by the tissue itself and not by the quantity of oxygen present. The view that by increasing the amount of oxygen offered to the tissue the intensity of the oxidations can likewise be increased was formerly held and is still met with. It is often supposed, for example, that by breathing pure oxygen the oxidations of the body may be augmented. On the contrary, the facts indicate that when a sufficient supply of oxygen is provided any further increase has no immediate effect in aiding or hastening the oxidations. The intensity of the process is conditioned by the tissue itself. The initial stimulus or substance that sets going the whole reaction arises within the tissues. The second generalization that seems to be accepted more and more of recent years is that the oxidations of the body, those reactions that give rise to much heat, do not affect the living tissue itself. They take place under the influence of the living matter, or by the aid of substances (enzymes) formed by the living matter, but the material actually burnt is not organized living substance. As the living yeast cells break down sugar in the liquid surrounding them, so the living tissue cells metab- olize and oxidize the dead food material contained in the lymph and tissue liquid in which they are bathed. The opposite point of view was ably advocated by Pfluger. This observer, in fact, ex- plained the mystery of physiological oxidations by assuming that the oxygen together with the food material is synthesized into the highly complex and unstable living molecules. The active intra- molecular movement within these molecules leads constantly to a breaking down, a splitting off of simpler molecules which consti- tute the products of physiological oxidation. The instability of the molecule is due to its size and the activity of the intramolecular movements, or, as Pfluger expressed it, "The intramolecular heat of the cell is its life." This point of view, however, has not found acceptance of late years. It is implied or stated by most recent authors that the food material is attacked and oxidized outside the living molecule, in the form of fat, sugar, protein, etc. The ten- dency for many years has been to show that these processes in the body are chemical changes that do not differ fundamentally from similar processes outside the body. The point of view actually adopted by most workers is that the living matter effects its won- derful changes in the food material with the aid of intracellular ferments or enzymes. That such enzymes are formed, one may say generally in the tissues of the body, has been brought out in the pre- ceding chapters upon Digestion and Nutrition. It is necessary only PHYSIOLOGICAL OXIDATIONS. 921 to recall the facts that lipase, the fat-splitting enzyme, has been iso- lated from many tissues, and that in the liver and muscles and prob- ably other tissues there exist enzymes capable of converting glycogen to sugar or the reverse, and of destroying the sugar completely by the serial action of several intracellular enzymes. Finally, with regard to the protein material it is now recognized that proteolytic enzymes are formed within many, if not all, of the living tissues. This point is demonstrated by the fact of autolysis, — that is, if living tissue is taken from the body, with precautions against contamination by bacteria, and while under perfect aseptic conditions is kept warm and moist, it will digest itself. The protein is split up into the same simple hyclrolytic products as are obtained by boiling it with acids. It has been shown that this digestion is due to enzymes — autolytic enzymes — formed within the living tissue. There is no doubt, therefore, of the existence of intracellular enzymes, and that these substances play a conspicuous part in the metabolism of food material. The lipase, the diastase, and the autolytic enzymes just referred to all belong to the group that cause hydrolytic cleavages, — that is, they induce splitting or decomposition of the material by a reaction with water. The supposition has naturally been made that probably the oxidations of the body are effected also by enzymes which in some way activate the oxygen. Enzymes of this char- acter have been found; they are designated in general as oxidases or as oxidases and peroxidases, the former term referring to those enzymes that effect oxidations in the presence of oxygen, while the latter is applied to certain enzymes supposed to act only in the presence of peroxids. According to Bach and Chodat the per- oxidases act upon organic compounds of the nature of peroxides, causing these latter to give off their oxygen in a form capable of producing oxidations. The peroxides are produced from certain organic substances which they designate as oxygenases. These latter take up ox}^gen and are converted to peroxides, as H20 may take up oxygen and be changed to H202. According to these authors all the oxidizing enzymes of the body are peroxidases, what have been called oxidases being simply a mixture of per- oxidases and oxygenase. Peroxidases have been described in the blood (Kastle and Amoss), in pus-cells, lymphoid tissue generally, and in sperm-cells (Czyhlarz and v. Fiirth). The most conspicuous of the oxidases or peroxidases found in the animal body is the one capable of oxidizing aldehydes (salicylic or benzoic aldehyde) into the corresponding acids, and hence designated specifically as aldehyclase. This enzyme has been extracted especially from the liver, lung, and spleen. An oxidase known as tyrosinase, first found in plant juice, has also been isolated from animal tissues. It oxidizes tyrosin with the formation of 922 NUTRITION AND HEAT REGULATION. homogentisinic acid. So also oxidases are described capable of oxidizing xanthin to hypoxanthin or to uric acid.* The process of destructive metabolism of sugar in the body, glycolysis, may be effected, it will be remembered, by the tissue juices squeezed from the organs, or even by extracts of the tissues of muscle, liver, etc. It may well be believed, therefore, that the oxidation of this most important food material is accomplished by the action of one or more enzymes. Such facts as these lend great probability to the belief that eventually it will be shown that the oxidations in the body are effected by the influence of oxidases or peroxidases acting singly, or in combination, or in sequence with the hydrolytic enzymes. While it is perfectly obvious that more facts are needed before positive statements are warranted regarding the chemistry of the oxidations in the body, the view entertained regarding the general process in the body is that the material — protein, fat, or carbohy- drate— is first split into simpler products by the action of a hy- drolytic enzyme, or a series of hydrolytic enzymes, formed in the cells. These reactions are not attended by any marked formation of heat. The split products thus produced are then acted upon by oxidases with the formation of certain intermediate products and eventually of carbon dioxid, water, etc., and the liberation of heat. A specific instance of this serial action has been given in reference to the oxidation of sugar (p. 869) : According to Stoklasa, the sugar is first split into lactic acid, and this into carbon dioxid and alcohol; the alcohol then by the action of a series of oxidases is oxidized to acetic acid, formic acid, carbon dioxid, and water. From our present standpoint, the production of heat in the body, it is impor- tant also to bear in mind the general view advocated by Speck and others — namely, that the chemical changes or metabolism of the body may be divided into two general classes: first, the heat-produc- ing or energy-liberating metabolism, which results finally in the oxidation of the great mass of the food material and which is essential for the production of body heat, and, second, the tissue metabolism proper — that is, the synthesis and disassimilation of the living substance itself. This latter metabolism varies probably in the different tissues; it is concerned with the building up and breaking down of the living machinery and is attended by the absorption as well as the liberation of heat, and the energy necessary for effecting these reactions is obtained from the heat energy liberated by the oxidation processes. In this last thought there is contained a suggestion which may serve as an explanation of the fundamental value of the physiological oxidations to the * For further details see < >ppenheimer, "Die Fermente und ihre Wir- kungen," second edition, V.W,, PHYSIOLOGICAL OXIDATIONS. 923 body. It may be supposed that these oxidations furnish not only the energy used in the work done generally by the cells but also the energy necessary for the anabolic or synthetic metabolism of living matter, that is to say, the construction of the complex and unstable protoplasm. In those organisms or cells that lead an anaerobic existence — that is, an existence in the absence of free oxygen — the energy necessary for the process is obtained perhaps from hydrolytic changes alone. SECTION IX. THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. With the exception of the phenomenon of consciousness, na fact of life excites more interest and seems to offer greater diffi- culties to an adequate explanation than the function of reproduc- tion. The male cell (spermatozoon) and the female cell (ovum) unite to form a new cell which thereupon begins to grow rapidly and produces an organism that in all of its manifold peculiarities of structure and function is essentially a replica of its parents. The fundamental problems presented in this act of reproduction are those of fertilization and heredity. In the former we must ascertain why the union of the two cells is necessary or advanta- geous, and the secret of the stimulating influence upon growth that arises from this union. Under the term heredity we express the obvi- ous, yet mysterious, fact that the fertilized ovum of each species de- velops into a structure like that of its parents. Both of these im- portant problems are essentially of a physiological character, — that is, they deal with properties of the living material composing the reproductive cells; but, at present, biological investigation along these lines is largely in the morphological stage. . The part of the sub- ject that can be studied with most success is the structural changes that are associated with fertilization and reproduction. Great, indeed wonderful, progress has been made during the last century, but it is needless perhaps to say that much remains unexplained, and that in this, as in so many other problems of nature, the greater our knowledge the clearer becomes our vision of the difficulties and complexities of a final scientific explanation. Outside these funda- mental problems there are other accessory functions connected, for instance, with the external genital organs which in a measure are of more immediate practical interest. In one way or another these functions are necessary or helpful to the final union of the repro- ductive cells. They form a part of the reproductive life which comes more immediately under our observation and control, and consti- tute, therefore, a subject which has been more accessible to in- vestigation. In the brief treatment given in the following chap- !< i more emphasis is laid upon this side, the accessory phenomena of reproduction, than upon the deeper, more fundamental prob- 92 I THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 925 lems, in view of the fact that the accessory phenomena are the ones which have at present the greater practical interest. The function of reproduction is often omitted from physiolog- ical courses, and the reason perhaps is partly that the structural features and the development of the embryo have been assigned to the department of anatomy, and partly because it is a function not essential to the maintenance of the existence and reactions of the organism. The reproductive organs might be eliminated en- tirely and the power of the body as an organism to maintain its individual existence not be seriously interfered with. The physio- logical importance of the reproductive organs lies not in their co-operation in the communal life of the various parts of the body, but in their adaptation to produce another similar being. We may explain, therefore, the co-ordinating mechanisms of the body without reference to the reproductive tissues, except so far as their supposed internal secretions affect general or specific metabolism. CHAPTER LII. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. The Graafian Follicle and the Corpus Luteum. — The functional value of the ovary is connected with the formation and rupture of the Graafian follicles, whereby an ovum is liberated. The pri- mordial follicles consist of an ovum surrounded by a layer of fol- licular epithelium. Beginning at a certain time after birth and continuing throughout the period of active sexual life, some of these primordial follicles develop into mature Graafian follicles and mi- grate to the surface of the ovary. The change consists in a pro- liferation of the follicular epithelium and the formation of a serous liquid, the liquor folliculi, between the layers of this epithelium. In the matured follicle there is a connective tissue covering, the theca folliculi, formed from the stroma of the ovary and consisting of two coats or tunics — the external and the internal. The cells in the internal tunic develop a yellowish pigment as the follicle grows, and are sometimes designated as lutein cells. Within the capsule formed by the internal tunic there is a layer of follicular cells known as the membrana granulosa and attached to one side is a mass of the same cells, the discus proligcrus — within which the ovum is imbedded. The follicular liquid lies between. This liquid increases in amount, and when the follicle has reached the 926 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. surface it forms a vesicle projecting to the exterior. This projecting portion is nearby bloodless and thinner than the rest of the wall of the follicle. It is designated as the stigma. When fully mature the follicle ruptures at the stigma and the egg, together with the sur- rounding follicular cells of the discus proligerus and a portion of the- membrana granulosa, is extruded, the egg being received into the open end of the Fallopian tube. According to Clark,* the rupture of the follicle is brought about by an increasing vascular congestion of the ovary. The tension within the ovary is thereby increased, the follicle is forced to the surface, and the circulation at the most projecting portion is interfered with to such an extent as to cause necrotic changes at the stigma, atwhich rupture finally occurs. After the bursting of the follicle its walls collapse, and the central cavity receives also some blood from the ruptured vessels of the theca. Later on the vesicle becomes filled with cells containing a yellow pigment. These cells increase rapidly and form a festooned border of increasing thickness around the central blood clot. The vesicle at this stage, on account of the yellow color of the new cells, is known as a corpus luteum. The structure thus formed increases in size for a period and then undergoes retrogressive changes and is finally completely absorbed. The duration of the period of growth and retrogression varies according as the egg liberated becomes fertilized or not. If fertilization does not occur, as is the case in the usual monthly periods, the corpus luteum reaches its maximum size within two to three weeks and then begins to be absorbed. It is frequently designated under these circumstances as the false corpus luteum (corpus luteum spurium) or corpus luteum of men- struation. In case the egg is fertilized and the woman becomes pregnant the life history of the corpus luteum is much prolonged. Instead of undergoing absorption after the third week it continues to increase in size by multiplication of the lutein cells during the first few months of pregnancy, and does not show retrogressive changes until the sixth month or later. The total size of the corpus in such cases is much larger than in menstruation, and it was des- ignated, therefore, by the older writers as the true corpus luteum (corpus luteum verum) or corpus luteum of pregnancy. Later observers agree that there is no essential difference in structure between the true and the false corpus luteum, although the former has a longer history and attains a greater size. The point of greatest structural interest in the corpus luteum is the origin of the yellow (lutein) cells. Histologists have been and still are divided upon this point; some believe that they arise from the cells of the membrana granulosa, others that they come from the connective tissue cells in the internal capsule (theca interna) of the follicle. * Clark, "Johns Hopkins Hospital Reports," 7, 181, 1898. THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 927 The majority of writers seem to favor the latter view.* Regarding the physiological importance of the corpus opinions also differ. Some regard it as simply a protective mechanism by means of which the empty space in the follicle is filled up by a tissue which is afterward easily absorbed, instead of by scar tissue. Others, how- ever, attribute to the lutein cells secretory functions of the most important character in connection with the subsequent develop- ment of the egg and the activities of the uterus. Some reference will be made to these views farther on. Menstruation. — The attainment of sexual maturity or puberty is marked by a number of visible changes in the body, but in the female the characteristic change is the appearance of the men- strual flow from the uterus. The age at which this phenomenon occurs shows many individual variations, but the average for temperate climates is given usually at 14 to 15 years. In the warmer countries the age is earlier, — 8 to 10 years, — and in the cold regions somewhat later, — 16 years. The racial characteristic in this respect is said to be maintained, however, after generations of residence in countries of a different climate, as is illustrated by the relatively early appearance of menstruation among Jews even in the colder countries. After the phenomenon appears it occurs at regular intervals of 28 days, more or less, and hence is known as the monthly period, menses, menstruation, or catamenia. The interval is not absolutely regular, and shows many individual variations within limits which may be placed at 20 to 35 days. Absence of the menstrual flow is designated as a condition of amen- orrhea. Certain premonitory symptoms usually precede the appearance* of the menses, such as pains in the back or head or a general feeling of discomfort, although in some cases these symp- toms are absent. When these premonitory symptoms are unusually painful or serious and the flow is difficult or irregular the condition is designated as dysmenorrhea. The flow begins with a discharge of mucus, which later becomes mixed with blood. The quantity of blood lost is subject to individual variations, but it may amount to as much as 100 to 200 gms. The flow continues for 3 or 4 days and then subsides. Under normal conditions this phenomenon occurs regularly throughout sexual life, — that is, during the period in which conception is possible. If fertilization occurs the flow ceases normally during pregnancy and the period of lactation. At the forty-fifth to the fiftieth year the flow disappears permanently, and this change marks what is known as the natural menopause, climacteric, or change of life. The change is sometimes abrupt, sometimes very gradual, being preceded by irregularities in * For discussion and literature see Marshall, "Quarterly Journal Micro- scopical Science," 1905, xlix., 189; and Loeb, "Journal of the American Medical Association," 1906, xlvi., 416. 928 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. menstruation, and it is not infrequently associated with psychical and physical disturbances of a serious character. If at any time during sexual life the ovaries are completely removed by surgical operation menstruation is brought to a close, this condition being designated as artificial menopause. Structural Changes in the Uterus During Menstruation. — Men- struation is a phenomenon of the uterus. The lining mucous mem- brane, the endometrium, in the period of four or five days preceding the flow, becomes rapidly thicker and its superficial layers are con- gested with blood, and indeed in places small collections of blood may be noticed. Opinions differ very much as to the change under- gone by this thickened membrane during the flow. According to some authors, most of the membrane is thrown off and the blood escapes from the denuded surface mixed with pieces of the mem- brane. According to others, no material destruction of the mem- brane occurs, the blood that escapes being due to small capillary extravasations or perhaps mainly to a process of diapedesis. It would seem that the amount of destruction of the endometrium must be subject to individual variations. After the cessation of the flow the mucous membrane is rapidly repaired by regenerative changes in the tissues; the surface epithelium, if denuded, is re- placed by proliferation of the cells lining the uterine glands and the thickened, edematous condition of the membrane rapidly sub- sides during a period of six or seven days. While the escape of blood takes place only from the surface of the uterus, the other reproductive organs — the ovary, the Fallopian tubes, and even the external genital organs — share to some extent in the vascular con- gestion exhibited by the uterus during the period preceding the menstrual flow. The mucous membrane of the uterus may be said to exhibit a constantly recurring menstrual cycle which falls into four periods: (1) Period of growth in the few (5) days preceding menstruation, characterized by a rapid increase in the stroma, blood-vessels, epithelium, etc., of the membrane. (2) The men- struation or period of degeneration (4 days), during which the capillary hemorrhage takes place and the epithelium suffers de- generative changes and is cast off more or less. (3) The period of regeneration (7 days), during which the mucous membrane returns to its normal size. (4) The period of rest (12 days), during which the endometrium remains in a quiescent condition. The Phenomenon of Heat (CEstrus) in Lower Mammals. — The phenomenon known as heat in lower mammals resembles, in many essential respects, menstruation in human beings, and they may be regarded as homologous functions. Heat is a period of sexual excitement which occurs one or more times during the year and during which the female will take the male. The condition THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 929 lasts, as a rule, for several days, and in the female is accompanied by changes which resemble those of menstruation. The external genital organs become swollen and in many animals there is a discharge of mucus or mucus and blood from the uterus. His- tologically the mucous membrane of the uterus undergoes changes similar to those of menstruation — that is, the membrane increases in size and becomes congested with blood — and it exhibits a phase of degeneration during which some of the epithelial lining may be cast off and some hemorrhage occur. As in the case of the men- strual period, the heat period or cestrous cycle may be divided into four subperiods (Marshall and Jolly) : the procestrum, during which the genital organs are congested and bleeding occurs, corresponds with menstruation; the oestrus, the period of sexual desire; the metoestrum, the period of repair and return to normal conditions, and the anoestrum, the period of rest. If sexual union is prevented during this period heat passes away in a few days, but recurs again at intervals which vary in the different mammals: 4 weeks in the monkey, mare, etc.; 3 to 4 weeks in the cow; 2\ to 4 weeks in the sheep; 9 to 18 days in the sow; 12 to 16 weeks in the bitch, etc. The recurrence of the period under these circumstances suggests at once the essential resemblance to the monthly periods of women. According to Heape's most interesting observations upon monkeys (Semnopithecus),* some of these animals show a regular monthly flow lasting for 4 days, except when conception takes place. The changes during heat must be considered as physiologically ho- mologous to those of menstruation. The sexual excitement that attends the condition in the lower animals is not distinctly repre- sented in man, although it is commonly said that in the period pre- ceding or following menstruation the sexual desire is stronger than at other times, but in the changes undergone by the uterus and the fact that these changes are connected, as a rule, with the liberation of an egg from the ovary (ovulation), the two phenomena are physiologically similar. Relation of the Ovaries to Menstruation. — It appears to be clearly demonstrated that the phenomenon of menstruation is de- pendent upon a periodical activity in the ovaries. When the ovaries are completely removed menstruation ceases (artificial menopause) and the uterus undergoes atrophy. When the ovaries are congenitally lacking or rudimentary, a condition of amenorrhea also exists. These facts and the connection of the ovaries with menstruation are further corroborated in a striking way by experi- ments upon transplantation or grafting of the ovary. This experi- ment has been performed upon lower animals (apes) as well as upon *Heape, "Philosophical Transactions, Royal Society," 185 (B), 1894, and 188 (B), 1897. 59 930 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. human beings. Removal of both ovaries in apes is followed by a ces- sation of menstruation. Transplantation of an ovary under the skin serves to maintain menstruation, but if subsequently removed this function disappears.* In the human being Morris and Glass ob- tained similar results. f An ovary or a piece of an ovary trans- planted into the uterus itself or the broad ligament caused a re- turn of the menstrual periods which had ceased after surgical re- moval of the glands, or brought on free menstruation in conditions of amenorrhea or dysmenorrhea. Man}- views have been proposed to explain this relationship between ovary and uterus. In most cases it has been assumed that the menstruation in the uterus is connected with the act of ovulation, — that is, the ripening and discharge of a Graafian follicle. Gynecologists, it is true, have accumulated facts to show that ovu- lation may occur independently of menstruation, but, as a rule, the two acts occur together, not simultaneously, but in a definite sequence, and the significance of menstruation is to be found in its physiological connection with the fate of the ovum. It was believed at first that the processes in the ovary influence the uterus by a nervous reflex. This view finds its most complete expression in the theory formulated by Pfliiger. According to this physiologist, the congestion of the uterus which leads to menstrua- tion and the congestion of the ovary which leads to ovulation are both reflex vasodilator effects due to the mechanical stimulation of the sensory nerves of the ovary by the growth in size of the fol- licle. As this structure develops the mechanical stimulus increases in intensity, the congestion in both organs becomes more pro- nounced and leads finally to the bursting of the follicle and the hemorrhage in the uterus. This very attractive theory does not, however, accord with the facts. Goltz and Reinf have shown by experiments upon dogs that when the nerves going to the uterus are completely severed from their central connections the animals can be fertilized, become pregnant, and give birth to a litter of young. Moreover, the experiments upon transplantation referred to above seem to show quite conclusively that a nervous connection is not essential to the influence that the ovary exerts upon the uterus. The present view, therefore, is that this influence is exerted through the blood, — the other great system connecting the organs with one another. The usual assumption is that the ovaries form an internal secretion which is given to the blood or lymph and upon reaching the uterine tissues serves to stimulate the mucous mem- brane to a more active growth. This theory has been elaborated * Halban, "Deutsche Gesellschaft f. Gynakol, " 9, L901. tGlass, "Medical News," 523, L899; Morris, "Medical Record," 83, VjOl. X Rein " Archiv I', die gesammte Physiologic," vol. xxiii. THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 931 most fully perhaps by Fraenkel,* who believes that this internal secretion is furnished by the yellow cells of the corpus luteum. This observer, from the results of operations upon women, believes that the ovum is normally discharged two weeks before menstrua- tion, and the resulting increased activity of the cells of the corpus luteum is responsible for the secretion which stimulates the uterus to the augmented growth that takes place in the premenstrual period. Whether or not the monthly change in the endometrium is directly dependent upon an internal secretion from the ovary or is an independent cyclic process peculiar to this tissue, there seems to be no doubt that the physiological integrity of the uterus as a whole is dependent upon the ovaries. Removal of the ovaries in the young prevents the normal development of the uterus, while removal in the adult causes a degeneration of the uterus, which, however, can be averted by a successful transplantation of ovarian tissue, f In the lower animals Marshall and Jolly J have been able to show that extracts of the ovaries, taken from an animal in or just before heat (procestrous or cestrous period), when injected into an animal during the ancestrum bring on a transient condition of heat. These authors do not believe, how- ever, that the chemical stimulus (hormone) formed in the ovary is developed by the cells of the corpus luteum, since according to their observation on cats and dogs ovulation does not occur until after heat has begun (prooestrum). The Physiological Significance of Menstruation. — Naturally many views have been proposed to explain the significance of men- struation. According to the Mosaic law, it is a process of purifica- tion; others have seen in it a mechanism to remove an excess of nutriment in the body; but since the period in which our knowl- edge of the structure of the organs concerned and of the histo- logical changes during the act became more definite, theories of the meaning of menstruation have usually assumed that it is a prepara- tion for the reception of the fertilized ovum. These views have taken two divergent forms according as the act of ovulation was believed to precede or to happen simultaneously with or subse- quently to the act of menstruation. According to one view, the swelling and congestion of the membrane constitute a prepara- tion for the reception of the fertilized ovum. If the ovum fails of fertilization, then degenerative changes ensue, and the membrane * Fraenkel, "Archiv f. Gynakologie, " 68,2, 1903. See also Ihm, " Monatssehrif t f. Geburtshiilfe u. Gynakol.," 21, 515, 1905, for discussion and extensive literature. f Carmichael and Jolly, "Proc. Roy. Soc.," B, 79, 1907, and Marshall and Jolly, "Roy. Soc. Edinb.," 45, 589, 1907. {Marshall and Jolly, "Philosophical Transactions, Royal Society," London, 1905, B. cxcviii., 99. 932 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. or a portion of it is cast off in the menstral flow, while the re- mainder is absorbed. According to this view, menstruation is an indication that fertilization has not taken place.* This view falls in with the belief that ovulation normally precedes menstrua- tion by a considerable interval. A modification or extension of this general hypothesis is proposed by Bryce and Teacher. f They believe that the process of menstruation is a cyclic one, which has for its object the preparation of the endometrium for the reception of the ovum. The monthly regeneration keeps this membrane in that condition of youthful irritability which enables it to respond promptly to the stimulus of the ovum by the formation of a decidua. The other point of view was advo- cated especially by Pfliiger in connection with his theory of a common cause of ovulation and menstruation. He assumed that menstruation occurs before the ovum reaches the uterus and that its physiological value lies in the fact that a raw surface is thus made upon which the ovum is grafted. Menstruation, according to him, is an operation of nature for the grafting of the fertilized ovum upon the maternal organism. This view finds considerable support in the fact that in some of the lower animals (dogs) the flow of blood (procestrum) precedes fertilization. The Effect of the Menstrual Cycle on Other Functions. — It is natural to suppose that such marked changes as occur in the ovary and uterus during the menstrual cycle should have an in- fluence upon other parts of the body. As a matter of fact, it is known that in general the sense of well-being varies with the phases of the cycle. At the time of or in the period just preceding the menstrual flow there is usually a more or less marked sense of ill- being or despondency, and a diminution in general efficiency. Among the various observations made by objective methods upon the functions of the different organs during these periods the most significant, probably, are those upon blood-pressure. According to Mosher,J the blood-pressure falls at the time of the menstrual periods. The curves obtained in these experiments are not entirely regular, but at or near the menstruation the blood-pressure falls .slowly, the maximum fall being coincident with the appearance of the flow. It would seem probable that the fall of general blood- pressure is due directly to the vascular dilatation in the genital or- gans and in turn is responsible for some of the secondary phenomena observed in the organism as a whole. It is very suggestive to find thai ilic author quoted above obtained similar periodical falls in * This view finds expression in the aphorisms: ['Women menstruate becau e they do no1 conceive," Powers, and "The menstrual crisis is the physiological bomologue of parturition," Jacobi. t Bryce and Teacher, " Early Developmenl and Imbedding of the Human Ovum,"' L908. % Mosher, "The Johns Hopkins Hospital Bulletin," 1901. THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 933. blood-pressure in men, suggesting the idea that has frequently been expressed, that in man as well as woman there is a rhythmical activity of the genital organs, a reproductive cycle that in man may be referred to the development and extrusion of the sperma- tozoa in the testis, as in woman it is connected with the growth and expulsion of the ova in the follicles of the ovary. The Passage of the Ovum into the Uterus. — The means by which the ovum gains entrance to the Fallopian tubes has given rise to much speculation and some interesting experiments. It was formerly believed (Haller) that at the time of ovulation the fimbriated end of the Fallopian tube is brought against the ovary, the movement being due to a congestion or a sort of erection of the fimbriae. This movement has not been observed, and, as experi- ments show that small objects introduced into the pelvic cavity are taken up by the tubes, it is believed that the cilia upon the fimbriae and in the tubes may suffice to set up a current that is sufficient to direct the movements of the ovum. Attention has been called to the fact that in animals with a bicornate uterus the ova may be liberated from the ovary on one side, as shown by the presence of the corpora lutea, while the embryos are developed in the horn of the other side. As further evidence for the same possibility of migration it has been shown that the ovary may be excised on one side and the horn of the uterus on the other and yet the animal may become pregnant after sexual union. It would seem probable, therefore, that the ovum is discharged into the pelvic cavity and may be caught up by the ciliary movements at the end of the tube on the same side, or may traverse the pelvic cavity in the narrow spaces between the viscera and be received by the tube on the other side. Such a view explains the possible occurrence of true abdominal pregnancies, and suggests also the possibility that ova may at times fail to reach the uterus at all and may undergo de- struction and absorption in the abdominal cavity. In some of the lower animals — the dog, for example — provision is made for the more certain entrance of the ova into the tubes b3^ the fact that the latter end in connection with a membranous sac of peri- toneum which envelopes the ovary. The sexual fertilization of the ovum is supposed to take place shortly after its entrance into the Fallopian tube, since spermatozoa have been found in this region, and the fertilized ovum, before reaching the seat of its im- plantation in the body of the uterus, has begun its development. By the act of coitus the spermatozoa are deposited at the mouth of the uterus, whence they make their way toward the tubes, being guided in their movements very probably by the opposing force of the ciliary contractions in the uterus. It is known that the cilia of the tubes and uterus contract so as to drive inert ' =y=i= — — d " -n ./ / 934 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. objects toward the vagina and they carry the egg in this direction, but the spermatozoa, being moved by the contractions of their own cilia or tails, are stimulated to advance against this ciliary current. The act of fertilization of the ovum is preceded by certain preparatory changes in the ovum itself which are described under the term maturation. Maturation of the Ovum. — The process of maturation occurs before or just after the spermatozoon enters the ovum. At the time the latter is extruded from the follicle it is a single cell sur- rounded by a layer of fol- licular epithelium forming the corona radiata, which is subsequently lost. The egg proper consists of cyto- plasm and a nucleus or germinal vesicle containing a nucleolus or germinal spot. Within the cyto- plasm is a definite collec- tion of food material or yolk which is sometimes designated as deutoplasm. The whole structure is sur- Fig. 292.— Human ovum (Lee, modified from rOUllded by a membrane Nagel): n, Nucleus (germinal vesicle) containing l™nwT1 „c + U„ 7nn!, rnrlinfl tne ameboid nucleolus (germinal spot); d, deu- KllOWn as Xtie ZOna raUlOXa toplasmic zone; p, protoplasmic zone; z, zona /Tr^o- 9Q9\ T-iofnvp nv nfrpr racliata; s, perivitellin space. \r l&- ***)• J^eiOie Ol dl lei the egg reaches the Fal- lopian tube its nucleus undergoes the changes preparatory to a mitotic division. The changes that occur in an ordinary cell division are represented schematically in Fig. 293. The nucleus at first presents the ordinary chromatin network, and in the cytoplasm lies the minute structure known as the centrosome. 'This latter divides into two daughter-centrosomes (b) which move to opposite sides of the nucleus and become surrounded by rays, each centrosome with its radiating system forming an astro- sphere. The chromatin material in the nucleus meanwhile has collected into larger threads known as chromosomes (c), and the nuclear membrane disappears (d). The number of chromo- somes is definite for each species of animal. The chromosomes arrange themselves equatorially between the astrospheres and then ca'-h divides Longitudinally into two parts (/). These parts migrate or are drawn toward their respective centrosomes (g, h, i), and this division is followed by a separation of the cytoplasm into two parts. Thus, two daughter-cells are formed, each containing the same number of chromosomes as the parent cell, but only half the Fig. 293. — Schematic representation of the processes occurring during cell division.- CBoveri.) THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 935 amount of chromatin material. The cell division results in a quantitative reduction of the chromatin material. In ordinary cell division the chromosomes again form a resting reticulum and a nuclear membrane and the chromatin substance increases in quantity. In the ovum during maturation two successive cell- divisions occur which resemble the typical cell-division just described, except that the daughter-cells are of very unequal size and that they contain each only half the normal number of chromosomes. In the first division, known sometimes as the heterotypical division, the process is preceded by a fusion of the chromosomes in pairs. In the division that ensues the pairs of chromosomes are split, one part going to each cell, with the result that each of the latter now contains half the number of chromo- somes— and each chromosome is an entire one from the parent cell, instead of half a one, as in the usual cell division. The two resulting cells are of very unequal size, the larger one is designated still as the ovum, the smaller one as the first polar body. The ovum now divides again (homotypical division), throwing off a second polar body. In this division the chromosomes, according to some observers, divide transversely, according to others, they divide longitudinally as in typical cell division.* In the formation and extrusion of the two polar bodies the matured ovum has suf- fered a quantitative and perhaps a qualitative reduction in chroma- tin material, and is left with only half its number of chromosomes. Since the first polar body after its separation may again divide' into two cells, the process of maturation results in the formation of four cells, three of which are polar bodies and may be regarded as abor- tive ova. The fourth, the matured ovum, retains practically all of the original cytoplasm, but has lost a part of its chromatin material and, according to Boveri, also its centrosome. The production of these four cells may be represented, therefore, by a schema of the kind shown in Fig. 294. The details of this process of forma- tion of the polar bodies and of reduction in chromatin material differ somewhat in different animals, f The process has not been followed in the human ovum, but since it occurs in the eggs of all animals with sexual reproduction, so far as they have been studied, it is justifiable to assume that a similar change takes place in man. From a biological standpoint the reduction of chromosomes and the loss of the centrosome, or of the power to produce a centrosome, throw much light upon the significance of fertilization by the male cell. The spermatozoon before it is ripe undergoes a process of maturation essentially similar to that described for the ovum. * For further details see Bryce in Embryology, " Quain's Anatomy," 1908. t For details see Wilson, " The Cell in Development and Inheritance," second edition, 1900, New York. 936 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTIOX. Two cell divisions take place with the formation of four spermatozoa, each of which, however, is a functional spermatozoon. In the pro- cess of division the number of chromosomes in each cell is reduced —-First polar doqv Mature egg vS^li T • * Abortive ova resulting from division of first polar body. Second polar body (abortive ovum). Fig. 294. — Schema to indicate the process of maturation of the ovum. — (Boveri.) by half. When the matured ovum and the matured spermatozoon fuse, therefore, each brings half the normal number of chromosomes, and the resulting fertilized ovum is a cell with its chromosomes restored to their usual number. The chromatin material is the essential part of the reproductive element. We have reasons to believe, in fact, that it is the substance which has the power of development and which conveys the hereditary structure specific to the animal. The process which causes each element to lose a part of this material before its union with the cell of the opposite sex is a provision by means of which the fertilized egg, from which the offspring develops, shall inherit the characteristics of each parent, without increase in the typical amount of the chromosomes. The loss of the centrosome by the matured egg is interpreted by Boveri as follows:* This minute structure is the instrument by which the mechanical process of cell division is initiated and completed. Its loss by the matured ovum prevents this cell from further develop- ment, but in the act of fertilization the spermatozoon brings into the egg a new centrosome, or causes the formation of a new centro- some after its entrance, and thus initiates a process of cell mul- tiplication. From this standpoint the loss of the centrosome by the egg is a provision to necessitate sexual union, and thus insure the benefits that presumably are associated with the fusion of two cells originating from different individuals. Fertilization of the Ovum. — The spermatozoon comes into contact with the ovum probably at the beginning of the Fallopian tubes. The meeting of the two cells is possibly not simply a matter of accidental contact, although the number of spermatozoa dis- charged by the male at coitus is so great that there would seem * For :i popular presental ion sec Boveri, " Dun Problem der Befruchtung," Jena, 1902. Fig. 295. — Schematic representation of the processes occurring during the fertiliza- tion and subsequent segmentation of the ovum. — (Boveri.) The chromatin (chromo- somes) of the ovum is represented in blue, that of the spermatozoon in red. THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 937 to be little chance for the ovum to fail to meet some of them. Ex- periments upon the reproductive elements of plants indicate, how- ever, that the egg may contain substances which serve to attract the spermatozoon, within a certain radius, by that force which is described under the name of chemotaxis. However this may be, the egg unites with a spermatozoon and under normal conditions with only one. A number of the spermatozoa may penetrate the zona radiata, but so soon as one has come into contact with the cytoplasm of the egg a reaction ensues in the surface layer which makes it impervious to other spermatozoa. The spermato- zoon consists of three essential parts, — the head, the middle piece, and the tail. The last named is the organ of locomotion, and after the spermatozoon enters the egg this portion atrophies and disappears, probably by absorption. The head of the spermato- zoon represents the nucleus, and contains the valuable chromatin material. On entering the egg it moves toward the nucleus of the latter, meanwhile enlarging and taking on the character of a nu- cleus. The egg now contains two nuclei, — one belonging to it origi- nally, the female pronucleus; one brought into it by the sperma- tozoon, the male pronucleus. The two come together and fuse, — superficially at least, — forming the nucleus of the fertilized egg, or the segmentation nucleus. The middle piece of the spermatozoon also enters the egg, but its exact function and fate is still a matter of some uncertainty. Boveri believes that it brings into the egg a centrosome or material which induces the formation of a centro- some in the ovum and is therefore of the greatest importance in initiating the actual process of cell division which begins promptly after the fusion of the nuclei. In the segmentation nucleus the nor- mal number of chromosomes is restored, and it is believed that in the subsequent divisions of the cell to form the embryo the chromo- somes are so divided that each cell gets some maternal and some paternal chromosomes, and thus shares the hereditary characteris- tics of each parent. This view is represented in a schematic way by Fig. 295, taken from Boveri, the maternal and paternal chromo- somes being indicated by different colors. According to this descrip- tion, both egg and spermatozoon are incomplete cells before fusion. The egg contains a nucleus and a large cell body, cytoplasm, rich in nutritive material, but it lacks a centrosome or the conditions neces- sary for the formation of an astrosphere, so that it cannot mul- tiply. The spermatozoon has also chromatin for a nucleus, and a centrosome or the material which may give rise to a centrosome, but it lacks cytoplasm — that is, food material for growth. It would seem that if the spermatozoon could be given a quantity of cytoplasm it would proceed to develop an embryo without the aid of an ovum. This experiment has, in fact, been made by 938 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. Boveri. Eggs of the sea-urchin were shaken violently so as to break them into fragments. If now a spermatozoon entered one of these fragments, which consisted only of cytoplasm, cell multiplication began and proceeded to the formation of a larva. On the other hand, it would seem to be equally evident that if a centrosome was present in the egg or some influence could be brought to bear upon it to make it form a centrosome it would develop without a spermatozoon. In some animals eggs do nor- mally develop at times without fertilization by a spermatozoon (parthenogenesis), the eggs that have this property probably pre- serving their centrosomes. Loeb* has shown, however, in some most interesting experiments that certain eggs, especially those of the sea-urchin (Strongylocentrotus purpura tus), which normally develop by fertilization with spermatozoa, may be made to de- velop by physicochemical means. His latest method is to treat the egg for a minute or two with an acid (acetic, formic, etc.), which causes the formation of a membrane. They are then placed for a certain interval in a hypertonic sea water, made by adding sodium chlorid to ordinary sea water. They are then transferred to normal sea water and after an hour or so they begin to multiply and eventually develop into normal larvae. Similar although less complete results were obtained previously by Morgan. Implantation of the Ovum. — -After fertilization in the tube the ovum begins to segment and multiply, and meanwhile is carried toward the uterus, probably by the action of the cilia lining the tube. Upon reaching the cavity of the uterus it becomes attached to the mucous membrane, usually in the neighborhood of the fundus. The membrane of the uterus has become much thickened mean- while, and in this condition is known usually as the decidua. The portion to which the egg becomes attached is the decidua serotina, and it eventually develops into the placenta, the organ through which the maternal nutriment is supplied to the fetus. The ovum has made considerable progress in its development before reaching the uterus, having formed amnion and chorion, with chorionic villi. Some of the ectodermal cells in the chorion become specialized to form a group of trophoblastic cells which have a digestive action, and it is suggested that the activity of these cells enables the ovum to become attached to the decidual membrane and to hollow out spaces in which the chorionic papilla become inserted.f The further development of the egg into a fetus, the formation of the decidua graviditatis, and the placenta are anatomical features that need nol be described here. Detail of these structures will be found * Loeb, " University of California Publications," 2, pp. 83, 89, and 113, 1905. See also Wilson," "Archiv f. entwick. Mechanik," 12, L901. f See Minot,0' Transactions of the American Gynecological Society," 1904. THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 939 in works on anatomy, embryology, or obstetrics. On the phys- iological side it has been found that removal of the ovaries, or even destruction of the corpora lutea, shortly after pregnancy has begun brings the process to an end, while a similar operation later in pregnancy has no effect upon the developing fetus or the subsequent act of parturition. It seems, therefore, that the process of implan- tation of the ovum in the uterine mucous membrane and the devel- opment of a placenta are dependent in some way upon the ovaries. The apparent explanation of the connection is given in the hypothe- sis that the corpora lutea, during their rapid development at the beginning of pregnancy, give off an internal secretion which controls or influences in some essential way the processes connected with the fixing of the fertilized ovum.* The Nutrition of the Embryo — Physiology of the Placenta. 1 — At the time of fertilization the ovum contains a small amount of nutriment in its cytoplasm. The amount, however, in the mam- malian ovum is small and suffices probably only for the initial stages of growth. When the ovum becomes implanted in the decidual membrane of the uterus the new material for growth must be ab- sorbed directly from the maternal blood of the uterus. Within a short time, however, the chorionic villi begin to burrow into the uterine membrane at the point of attachment, the decidua serotina, and the placenta gradually forms as a definite organ for the control of fetal nutrition. The details of histological structure of this organ must be obtained from anatomical sources. For the purposes of understanding its general functions it is sufficient to recall that the placenta consists essentially of vascular chorionic papilla? from the fetus bathed in large blood-spaces in the decidual membrane of the mother. The fetal and the maternal blood do not come into actual contact ; they are separated from each other by the walls of the fetal blood-vessels and the epithelial layers of the chorionic villi, but an active diffusion relation is set up between them. Nutritive material, protein, fat, and carbohydrate, and oxygen pass from the maternal to the fetal blood, and the waste products of fetal metabolism — carbon dioxid, nitrogenous wastes, etc., pass from the fetal to the maternal blood. The nutrition of the fetal tissues is maintained, in fact, in much the same way as though it were an actual part of the maternal organism. That material passes from the maternal to the fetal blood is a necessary inference from the growth of the fetus. The fact has also been demonstrated repeat- edly by direct experiment. Madder added to the food of the mother colors the bones of the embryo. Salts of various kinds, sugar, drugs, etc., injected into the maternal circulation may afterward be de- tected in the fetal blood. But we are far from having data that * Marshall and Jolly and Fraenkel, loc. cit. 940 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. would justify us in supposing that the exchange between the two bloods is effected by the known physical processes of os- mosis, diffusion, and filtration. The difficulties in understanding the exchange in this case are the same as in the absorption of nour- ishment by the tissues generally. It is perhaps generally assumed that the chorionic villi play an active part in the process, func- tioning, in fact, in much the same way as the intestinal villi. This assumption implies that the epithelial cells of the villi take an active part in the absorption of material by virtue of processes which can- not be wholly explained, but which without doubt are due to the chemical and physical properties of the substance of which they are composed. This assumption does not mean that the simpler and better understood physical properties of diffusion and osmosis are not also important. The respiratory exchange of gases, the diffusion of water, salts, and sugar, may be largely controlled in this way. There are no facts at least which contradict such an assump- tion. The passage of fats and proteins, however, would seem to require some special activity in the chorionic tissue, which may be connected with the presence of special enzymes. Glycogen occurs in the placenta itself and in all the tissues of the embryo during the period of most active growth. In the later period of embryonic life, as the liver assumes its functions, the glycogen becomes more localized to this organ and disappears, except for traces, in the skin, lungs, and other tissues in which it was present at first in considerable quantities. It would appear, therefore, that glycogen (sugar) represents one of the important materials for the growth of the embryo, and that in the beginning at least the tissues generally have a glycogenetic power. The sugar brought to the placenta in the maternal blood passes over into the fetal blood and the excess beyond that immediately consumed is deposited in the tissues as glycogen. The body fat of the fetus is at first slight in amount, but after the sixth month begins to increase with some rapidity. The fat-forming tissues are in full activity, therefore, before birth, and function doubtless in the same way as in the adult. Before birth also the various organs begin to take on their normal activity. The kidney may form urine long before birth, as is shown by the presence of this secretion in the bladder, and, shortly before birth at least, it has the power of producing hippuric acid, as may be shown by injecting benzoates into the blood of the mother. The kidney functions of the embryo, how- ever, are doubtless performed chiefly by the placenta and the kidney of the mother up to the time of birth. That the liver also begins to assume its functions early is shown by the fact that from the fifth to the sixth month one may find bile in the gall-bladder. In the intestine, colon, there is found also a collection of excrement, THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 941 the meconium, which shows that the motor and, secretory functions of the intestinal canal may be present in the last months of fetal life. From the pancreas a proteolytic enzyme may be extracted at the time of birth or before, but the amylolytic enzyme is not formed apparently until some time later. It is stated, at least, that it is not present at birth. In general, it is evident that for a long period the maternal organism digests and prepares the food for the embryo, excretes the wastes, regulates the conditions of temperature, etc., as it does for a portion of its own substance, but as the fetus ap- proaches term its tissues and organs begin to assume more of an independent activity, as indeed must be the case in preparation for the sudden change at birth. In this respect, as in all parts of the reproductive process, we meet with regulations whose mechanism is but dimly understood. Changes in the Maternal Organism during Pregnancy. — The two most distinct effects upon the mother that result from pregnancy are the growth of the uterus and of the mammary gland. The virgin uterus is small and firm, weighing from 30 to 40 gms., while at the end of pregnancy it may weigh as much as 1000 gms. This great increase in material is due partly to the growth of new muscular tissue and partly to an hypertrophy of the muscle already present. In the uterus at term the muscle cells are much longer and larger than in the organ before fertilization. The stimulus that initiates and controls this new growth is seemingly the fertil- ized ovum itself, but the physiological means employed are not comprehended. We know from experiments upon lower animals (Rein) that when all connections with the central nervous system are severed the fetus develops normally and the uterus increases correspondingly in size and weight. The influence of the ovum on the uterus must be exerted, therefore, either through some local nerve centers in the uterus, or, as seems much more probable, through some chemical stimulus which it gives to the organ. The effect of the presence and growth of the fetus on the mammary gland is treated in a separate paragraph below. In addition to these two visible effects it is evident that the growth of the fetus has an important influence on general metabolism and therefore upon the whole maternal organism. This fact is indicated by the marked changes often exhibited in the physical and mental con- dition of the mother. It is shown more precisely by a study of the nutritional changes. Numerous investigations have been made upon this subject, especially as regards the nitrogen equilibrium. During the latter part of pregnancy, especially, the nitrogen balance is positive — that is, nitrogen is stored as protein — due doubtless both to the growth of the embryo and the increase in material in the uterus and mammary gland. The proportion of ammonia in 942 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. the urine increases during pregnancy and especially during labor (Slemmons*). Parturition. — The fetus " comes to term " usually in the tenth menstrual period after conception — that is, about 280 days after the last menstruation. The actual time of delivery, however shows considerable variation. Delivery occurs in consequence of contractions, more or less periodical, of the musculature of the- uterus, and reflex as well as voluntary contractions of the abdom- inal muscles. It has been shown that delivery ma}r occur when the nerves connecting the uterus with the central nervous system are severed, so that the act is essentially an independent function of the uterus, although under normal conditions the contractions of this organ are doubtless influenced by reflex effects through its. extrinsic nerves. It has been shown that contractions of the gravid uterus may be caused by stimulation of various sensory nerves, and in women it is known that delivery may be precipitated prematurely by various mental or physical disturbances. The interesting prob- lem physiologically is to determine the normal factor or factors that bring on uterine contractions at term. Various more or less unsatis- factory theories have been proposed. Some authors attribute the act to a change in the maternal organism, such as mechani- cal distension of the uterus, a venous condition of the blood, a degenerative change in the placenta, etc., while others suppose that the initial stimulus comes from the fetus. In the latter case it is suggested that the increasing metabolism of the fetus is insuffi- ciently provided for by the placental exchange, and that therefore certain products are formed which serve to stimulate the uterus to contraction. The duration of the labor pains is variable, but usually they are longer in primiparse, ten to twenty hours or more, than in multip- aras. After the fetus is delivered the contractions of the uterus continue until the placenta also is expelled as the "after-birth." During these latter contractions the fetal blood in the placenta is, for the most part, squeezed into the circulation of the new-born child. The hemorrhage from the walls of the uterus due to the rup- ture of the placenta may be profuse at first, but under normal con- ditions is soon controlled by the firm contraction of the uterine walls. The Mammary Glands. — At the time of puberty the mam- mary glands increase in size, but this growth is confined mainly to the connective tissue; the true glandular tissue remains rudi- mentary and functionless. At the time of conception the gland- ular tissue is in some way stimulated to growth. Secreting alveoli are formed, and during the latter part of pregnancy they produce an incomplete secretion, scanty in amount, known as colostrum- ■■ Slemmons, "The Johns Hopkins Hospital Reports," 12, ill, 1904. THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 943 After delivery the gland evidently is again brought under the influence of special stimuli. It becomes rapidly enlarged and a more abundant secretion is formed. For the first day or two this secretion still has the characteristics of colostrum, but on the third or fourth day the true milk is formed and thereafter is produced abundantly, during the period of lactation, under the in- fluence of the act of milking. If during this period a new con- ception occurs the milk secretion is altered in composition and finally ceases. On the other hand, if the act of nursing is aban- doned permanently the glands after a preliminary stage of turgid- ity undergo retrogressive changes that result in the cessation of secretory activity. The colostrum secretion that occurs during pregnancy and for a day or two after birth differs from milk in its composition and histological structure. It is a thin, yellowish liquid containing a larger percentage of albumin and globulin and a smaller percentage of milk-sugar and fat than normal milk. Under the microscope it shows, in addition to some fat droplets, certain large elements, — the colostrum corpuscles. These con- sist of spherical cells filled with fat droplets, and are most probably leucocytes filled with fat which they have ingested. Colostrum corpuscles may occur in milk whenever the secretion of the gland is interfered with, and their presence may be taken as an indi- cation of an incomplete secretion. The Connection Between the Uterus and the Mammary Gland. — The physiological connection between the uterus and the mammary gland is shown by the facts mentioned in the pre- ceding paragraph. That the ovary also shares in this influence either directly or through its effect on the uterus is shown by the fact that after complete ovariotomy the mammar}^ gland under- goes atrophy. This undoubted influence of one organ upon the other might be exerted either through the central nervous system or by way of the circulation. There are indications that the secretion of the mammary glands is under the control, to some extent at least, of the central nervous system. For instance, in women during the period of lactation cases have been recorded in which the secretion was altered or perhaps entirely suppressed by strong emotions, by an epileptic attack, etc. This indication has not received satisfactory confirmation from the side of ex- perimental physiology. Eckhard found that section of the main nerve-trunk supplying the gland in goats, the external spermatic, caused no difference in the quantity or qualit}r of the secretion. Rohrig obtained more positive results, inasmuch as he found that some of the branches of the external spermatic supply vasomotor fibers to the blood-vessels of the gland and influence the secretion of milk by controlling the local blood-flow in the gland. Section 944 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. of the inferior branch of this nerve, for example, gave increased secretion, while stimulation caused diminished secretion, as in the case of the vasoconstrictor fibers to the kidney. These results have not been confirmed by others — in fact, the}'' have been sub- jected to adverse criticism — and they cannot, therefore, be ac- cepted unhesitatingly. After apparently complete separation of the gland from all its extrinsic nerves, not only does the secretion, if it was previously present, continue to form, although less in quantity, but in opera- tions of this kind upon pregnant animals the glands increase in size during pregnancy and become functional after the act of parturi- tion.* This result confirms the older experiments of Goltz, Rein, and others, according to which section of all the nerves going to the uterus does not prevent the normal effect on lactation after delivery. Regarding the question of the existence of secretory nerves, Baschf reports that extirpation of the celiac ganglion or section of the spermatic nerve does not prevent the secretion, but causes the appearance of colostrum corpuscles. Experiments, therefore, as far as they have been carried in- dicate that the gland is under the regulating control of the cen- tral nervous system, either through secretory or more probably through vasomotor fibers. The bond of connection between the mammary gland and the uterus is, however, established mainly through the blood rather than through the nervous system. Some direct evidence for this point of view is furnished by the interesting experiments of Starling and Lane-Claypon.J These authors found that extracts made from the body of the fetus, or rather from the bodies of many fetuses, when injected repeatedly into a virgin rabbit caused a genuine development of the mammary glands closely simulating the growth that normally occurs during pregnancy. Since similar extracts made from ovaries, placental and uterine tissues had no effect, they conclude that a specific chemical sub- stance (a hormone) is produced in the fetus itself and, after absorp- tion into the maternal blood, it acts upon the mammary gland, stim- ulating it to growth. Since the birth of the fetus is followed by active secretion in the mammary glands they adopt further the view that this substance, while promoting the growth of the gland tissue, inhibits the catabolic processes which lead to the formation of the secretion. With the birth of the fetus this substance is withdrawn and secretion begins, and, on the contrary, the secretion is suspended when a new pregnancy is well advanced. *Mironow, "Archives des sciences biologiques, " St. Petersburg, 3, 353, 1894. t Basch, "Ergebnisse der Physiologie, " vol. ii., part i, 1903. + Lane-Claypon and Starling, "Proceedings of the Royal Society," 1906, B. lxxvii.; ee al o Starling in "Lancet," 1905. THE FEMALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 945 As was said in speaking of the histology of the gland, the se- creting alveoli are not fully formed until the first pregnancy. Dur- ing the period of gestation the epithelial cells multiply, the alveoli are formed, and after parturition secretion begins. As the liquid is formed it accumulates in the enlarged galactophorous ducts, and after the tension has reached a certain point further secretion is apparently inhibited. If the ducts are emptied, by the infant or otherwise, a new secretion begins. The emptying of the ducts, in fact, seems to constitute the normal physiological stimulus to the gland-cells, but how this act affects the secreting cells, whether reflexly or directly, is not known. Composition of the Milk. — The composition of milk is com- plex and variable.* The important constituents are the fats, held in emulsion as minute oil droplets, and consisting chiefly of olein and palmitin; casein, a nucleo-albumin which clots under the in- fluence of rennin; milk-albumin or lactalbumin, a proteid resem- bling serum-albumin; lactoglobulin; lactose or milk-sugar; lecithin, cholesterin, phosphocarnic acid, urea, creatin, citric acid, enzymes, and mineral salts. It is well known also that many foreign sub- stances—drugs, flavors, etc. — introduced with the food are secreted in the milk. An average composition is: proteins, 2 to 3 per cent.; fats, 3 to 4 per cent.; sugar, 6 to 7 per cent.; salts, 0.2 to 0.3 per cent. The fact that casein and milk-sugar do not exist preformed in the blood is an argument in favor of the view that they are formed by the secretory metabolism of the gland cells. The special com- position of the milk-fat and the histological appearance of the gland cells during secretion lead to the view that the fat is also constructed within the gland itself. Bunge has called attention to the fact that the inorganic salts of milk differ quantitatively from those in the blood-plasma and resemble closely the propor- tions found in the body of the young animal, thus indicating an adaptive secretion. This fact is illustrated in the following table giving the mineral constituents in 100 parts of ash: Young Pup. K20 8.5 Na,0 8.2 CaO 35.8 MgO 1.6 Fe203 0.3-1 P,05 39.8 CI 7.3 On account of the use of cows' milk in place of human milk in the nourishment of infants much attention has been given to * For data as to composition and hygienic relations, sec Bulletin 41, "Hygienic Laboratory," Public Health and Marine Hospital Service, U. S., Washington, 1908. 60 Dogs' Milk. Dogs' Serum. 10.7 2.4 6.1 52.1 34.4 2.1 1.5 0.5 0.14 0.12 37.5 5.9 12.4 47.6 946 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. the relative composition and properties of the two secretions. The chief difference between the two lies apparently in the casein. The casein of human milk is smaller in amount, curdles in looser flocks than that of cows' milk, and seems to dissolve more easily and completely in gastric juice. The former also contains rela- tively more lecithin and less ash, particularly the lime salts. On the other hand, cows' milk contains less sugar and fat. In using it, therefore, for the nutrition of infants it is customary to add water and sugar. The composition of cows' milk is so well known that it is easy to modify it for special cases according to the in- dications. The rules for this procedure will be found in works upon pediatrics. CHAPTER LIII. PHYSIOLOGY OF THE MALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. The sexual life of the male is longer than that of the female. Puberty or sexual maturity begins somewhat later, — in tem- perate climates at about the fifteenth year; but there is no dis- tinct limitation of the reproductive powers in old age correspond- ing to the menopause of the female. At the time of puberty and for a short preceding period the boy grows more rapidly in stature and weight, and the assumption of its complete functions by the testis exerts a general influence upon the organism as a whole. One of the superficial changes at this period which is very evident is the alteration in pitch of the voice. Owing to the rapid growth of the larynx and the vocal cords the voice becomes markedly deeper, and the change is in some cases sufficiently sudden to cause the well-known phenomenon of the breaking of the voice. The neuromuscular control of the vocal cords becomes for a time un- certain. The completion of puberty can not be determined in the boy with the same exactness as in the girl, in whom menstruation furnishes a visible sign of sexual maturity. Much of the sexual mechanism may be functional long before the time of puberty, as is shown by the presence of sexual desire and the possibility of erection; but fully developed spermatozoa are not produced until this period, and indeed the presence of ripe and functional spermatozoa in the testis is the only certain sign that sexual ma- turity has been attained. Puberty consists in the maturation of the testis in the male, and of the ovary in the female. The Properties of the Spermatozoa. — The development and maturation of the spermatozoa in the testis has been followed successfully by histological means. The mother-cells of the sper- matozoa, the spermatocytes, give rise to four daughter-cells, sper- matids, each of which develops into a functional spermatozoon. The process in this case is something more than mere cell division, since in the spermatozoa eventually produced the number of chromosomes present in the nucleus — that is, the head of the sper- matozoon— are reduced by one-half. The process of production of the spermatozoa is therefore quite analogous to the maturation of the ovum during the formation of the polar bodies. The forma- tion and maturation of the spermatozoa may be represented by 947 94S THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. a schema similar to that used in the case of the ova, as follows (Fig. 296) : In the case of the ovum four ova are produced, but only one is functional, and this one, the ripe egg, is characterized by its large amount of cytoplasm, its inability to undergo further cell division until fertilized, owing possibly to its loss of the cen- trosome, and the reduction of its chromosomes to half the num- ber characteristic of the body cells of the species. In the case of the spermatozoa, the four cells produced are all functional,* and are characterized by the practical loss of cytoplasm, reduc- tion of chromosomes by one-half, and inability to multiply until cell material is furnished. The two cells supplement each other, therefore. Their union restores the normal number of chromo- somes, part of which are now maternal and part paternal; the egg supplies the cytoplasm and the spermatozoon nuclear material and the definite stimulus that leads to multiplication. The spermatozoa are produced in enormous numbers. It is calculated that at ejaculation each cubic centimeter of the liquid contains from sixty to seventy millions of these cells. The adult ripe spermatozoon is characterized as an independ- ent cell by its great motility, due to the cilia-like contrac- tions of its tail. Its power of movement or its vitality is retained under favorable con- ditions for very long periods. The most striking instance of this fact is found in the case of bats. In these animals copulation takes place in the fall and the uterus of the female retains the spermatozoa in activity until the period of ovulation in the following spring. Even in the human being it is believed that the spermatozoa may exist for many days in the uterus and Fallopian tubes of the female. In the semen that is ejaculated during coitus the spermatozoa are mixed with the secretions of the accessory reproductive glands, such as the seminal vesicles, the prostate gland, and Cowper's gland. The specific in- fluence of each of these secretions is not entirely understood, but experiments show that in some way they are essential to or aid greatly in maintaining the motility of the spermatozoa. Steinachf has found, for example, that removal of the prostate gland and *It is an interesting fact, that in some cases (bees) two kinds of spermatids are formed by an unequal division of the spermatocyte, and the smaller of the two is abortive. ;is in the case of the polar bodies of the egg. t See Steinach, " Archivf. d. gesammte Physiologie," 56, 1894, and Walker, "Archiv f. Anatomie u. Physiologie," ISO!), j>. 313. Spermatids. - Spermatozoa. Fig. 296 — Schema to indicate the proc- ess of maturation of the spermatozoa. — (Boveri). THE MALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 949 seminal vesicles in white rats prevents successful fertilization of the female, although the ability and desire to copulate is not interfered with. Direct experiments show that the secretion of the prostate gland maintains motility much more efficiently than a solution of physiological saline. It seems certain that the secretions of the mu- cous membrane of the uterus and Fallopian tubes exercise a similar favorable influence. Kolliker and others have investigated the action of many substances upon the motilitj^ of the spermatozoa, such as acids, alkalies, salts of various kinds and in different con- centrations, sugar, ethereal oils, etc. The union of spermatozoon and ovum is believed to take place usually in the Fallopian tube, and under normal conditions only one spermatozoon penetrates into the egg. The remainder of the great number that may be present eventually perish. The changes that take place during the process of fertilization have already been described (p. 936). Chemistry of the Spermatozoa. — Much chemical work has been done upon the composition of spermatozoa, particularly in the fishes. The results have been most interesting from a chem- ical standpoint, and biologically they are suggestive in that the analytical work has been done upon the heads of the spermatozoa. These heads consist entirely of nuclear material, and contain the substance or substances which convey the hereditary characteristics of the father. Whatever progress may be made in the understand- ing of the chemistry of this material is a step toward the solution of the most difficult and mysterious side of reproduction, the power of hereditary transmission. Miescher, in investigations upon the spermatozoa of salmon, discovered that the heads are composed essentially of an organic combination of phosphoric acid, since designated as nucleic acid, united with a basic albuminous body, protamin. This view has been confirmed and extended by later observers, especially by Kossel and his pupils.* The head of the spermatozoon, the male pronucleus in fertilization, may be de- fined, in the case of the fishes at least, as "a salt of an organic base and an organic acid, a protamin-nucleic acid compound." The term protamin is used now to designate a group of closely related substances obtained from the spermatozoa of different animals. The special protamin of each species is designated ac- cording to the zoological name of that species; thus the protamin of salmon is salmin, of hering (Clupea harengus) clupein, and so on. The protamins are all strong bases; their aqueous solutions give an intense alkaline reaction, and they unite readily with various acids to form well-defined salts. They are albuminous bodies, giving the biuret reaction readily even without the addition of * For literature and details of the chemistry of spermatozoa see Burian, in ' Ergebnisse der Physiologie, " vol. iii., part I, 1904, and 1906, v., 832. 950 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. alkali, and they are precipitated by most of the general precipitants of proteins, such as the neutral salts, the alkaloidal reagents, etc. Their solutions, however, are not coagulated by heat. The molec- ular formula for salmin is given as C30H57N17O6. When decom- posed by the action of acids they yield simpler basic products, the so-called hexon bases or diamino-acids, and particularly the base arginin (C6H14N402), which is contained in the protamin of the spermatozoa in greater abundance than in any other protein. The protamins differ from most other protein compounds by their relative simplicity; they contain no cystin grouping, therefore no sulphur; no carbohydrate grouping in most of the compounds examined; and no tyrosin complex. In the spermatozoa of some fishes the protamins are replaced by more complex compounds belonging to the group of histons which show properties somewhat intermediate between those of protamins and ordinary proteins, and in general it may be said that the head of the spermatozoon, like the nuclei of cells in general, consists chiefly of a nucleoprotein compound, that is, a compound of nucleic acid with a protein body of a more or less distinctly basic character.* The nucleic acid com- ponent of the spermatozoon resembles the same substance as obtained from the nuclei of other cells. In the spermatozoa of the salmon this nucleic acid has the formula C40H56NuP4O26. On decomposition by hydrolysis it yields at first some of the purin bases (adenin, guanin), and on deeper cleavage a number of compounds, including the pyrimidin derivatives, thymin, uracil, and cytosin. While the chemical studies upon spermatozoa, thus briefly referred to, have greatly extended our knowledge, it is still impossible to say that they have given any information concerning the peculiar functions of the spermatozoa in fertilization. The Act of Erection. — In the sexual life of the male the act of erection of the penis during coitus offers a most striking physical phenomenon. During this act the penis becomes hard and erect, owing to an engorgement with blood. The structure of the corpora cavernosa and corpus spongiosum is adapted to this function, being composed of relatively large spaces inclosed in trabeculse of connec- tive and plain muscle tissue, — the so-called erectile tissue. Many theories have been proposed to explain the mechanism of erection, but it is generally agreed that the work of Eckhard f demonstrated the essential facts in the process. This investigator discovered that in the dog stimulation of the nervi erigentes causes erection. These nerves are composed of autonomic fibers arising from the sacral por- tion of the spinal cord (see Figs. 110 and 111). They arise from the * Burian, loc. dt. t Eckhard, "Beitrage zur Anatomie und Physiologie, " 3, 123, 1803, and 4, 69, 1869. THE MALE REPRODUCTIVE ORGANS. 951 sacral spinal nerves, first to third (dog), on each side and help to form the pelvic plexus. They contain vasodilator fibers to the penis, as well as to the rectum and anus, and also visceromotor fibers to the descending colon, rectum, and anus. Eckhard, Loven, and others * have shown that when these fibers are stimulated there is a large dilatation of the arterioles in the erectile tissue of the penis and a greatly augmented blood-flow to the organ. If the erectile tissue is cut or the dorsal vein is opened the blood-flow under usual con- ditions is a slow stream, but when the nervus erigens is stimulated the outflow is very greatly increased; according to Eckhard's measurements, eight to fifteen times more blood flows out of the organ. The act of erection is therefore due essentially to a vas- cular dilatation of the small arteries whereby the cavernous spaces become filled with blood under considerable pressure. The caver- nous tissues are distended to the limits permitted by their tough, fibrous wall. It seems probable that the turgidity or rigidity of the congested organ is completed by a partial occlusion of the venous outflow, which is effected by a compression of the efferent vein by means of the extrinsic muscles (ischio and bulbocavernosus) and possibly by the intrinsic musculature as well. This compres^ sion does not occlude the blood-flow completely, but serves to in- crease greatly the venous pressure. This explanation of the act of erection, while no doubt correct, so far as it goes, leaves undeter- mined the means by which the dilatation of the small arteries is produced. Vasodilator nerve fibers in general are assumed to pro- duce a dilatation by inhibiting the peripheral tonicity of the arterial walls. If this explanation is applied to the case under consideration it forces us to believe that throughout life, except for the very occasional acts of erection, the arteries in the penis are kept in a constant condition of active tone. Moreover, on this view we should expect that section of the vasoconstrictor fibers to the penis, by abolishing the tone of the arteries, would also cause erection. These constrictor fibers arise fiom the second to fifth lumbar spinal nerves, and reach the organ by way of the hypo- gastric nerve and plexus and the pudic nerve. No such result of their section is reported and it seems that in the matter of erec- tion the actual mechanism of the great dilatation caused by the nervi erigentes still contains some points that need investigation. The Reflex Apparatus of Erection and Ejaculaticn. — The dilatation of the arteries of the penis during erection is noimally a reflex act, effected through a center in the lumbar cord. This center may be acted upon by impulses descending from the brain, as in the case of erotic sensations, or by afferent impulses arising in *See especially Francois-Franck, "Archives de Physiol, norm, etpathol.," 1895, 122 and 138. 952 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. some part of the genital tract, — from the testes themselves, from the urethra or prostate gland, and especially from the glans penis. Mechanical stimulation of the glans leads to erection, and Eckhard showed in dogs that section of the pudic nerve prevents this reflex from occurring, proving, therefore, that the sensory fibers concerned run in the pudic nerve. Stimulation of these latter fibers leads also to erotic sensations and eventually to the completion of the sexual orgasm. This latter act brings about the forcible ejection of the sperm through the urethra. It is initiated by contractions of the musculature of the vasa deferentia, ejaculatory duct, the seminal vesicles, and the prostate gland, which force the spermatozoa, to- gether with the secretions of the vesicles and prostate gland, into the urethra, whence they are expelled in the culminating stage of the orgasm by the rhythmical contractions of the ischiocavernosus and bulbocavernosus muscles, together with the constrictor urethrse. The immediate center for this complex reflex is assumed to lie in the lumbar cord, since, according to the experiments of Goltz, mechanical stimulation of the glans in dogs causes erection and seminal emission after the lumbar cord is severed from the rest of the central nervous system. Under ordinary conditions the act is accompanied by strong psychical reactions which indicate that the cortical region of the cerebrum is involved. It is interesting in this connection to find that electrical stimulation of a definite re- gion in the cortex* of dogs may cause erection and ejaculation. * Pussep, quoted from Hermann's " Jahresbericht der Physiologie/ vol. xi, 1903. CHAPTER LIV. HEREDITY— DETERMINATION OF SEX— GROWTH AND SENESCENCE. Heredity. — The development of the fertilized ovum offers two general phenomena for consideration: First, the mere fact of mul- tiplication by which an infinite number of cells are produced by successive cell-divisions; second, the fact that these cells become differentiated in structure in an orderly and determinate way so as to form an organism of definite structure like those which gave origin to the ovum and the spermatozoon. In other words, the fertilized ovum possesses a property which, for want of a better term, we may designate as a form-building power. The ovum develops true to its species, or, indeed, more or less strictly in accord- ance with the peculiarities of structure characteristic of its parents. The object of a complete theory of heredity is to ascertain the me- chanical causes — that is, the physicochemical properties — resi- dent in the fertilized ovum which impel it to follow in each case a definite line of development. The discussions upon this point have centered around two fundamentally different conceptions designated as evolution and epigenesis. Evolution and Epigenesis. — The earlier embryologists found a superficial explanation of this problem in the view that in the germ cells there exists a miniature animal already preformed, and that its development under the influence of fertilization consists in a process of growth by means of which the minute organism is unfolded, as it were. The process of development is a process of evolution of a pre-existing structure. Inasmuch as countless in- dividuals develop in successive generations, it was assumed also that in the germ cell there are included countless miniature organ- isms,— one incased, as it were, in the other. Some of the embry- ologists of that period conceived that the undeveloped embryos are contained in the ovum, — the ovists, — while others believed that they are present in the spermatozoon, the animalculists. Other embryologists pointed out that the fertilized egg shows no indication of a preformed structure, and therefore concluded that development starts from an essentially structureless cell, and consists in the successive formation and addition of new parts which do not pre- 953 954 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. exist as such in the fertilized egg. This view in contradistinction to the evolution theory was designated as epigenesis. Microscopi- cal investigation has demonstrated beyond all doubt that 'the fer- tilized ovum is a simple cell devoid of any parts or organs resem- bling those of the adult, and the evolution theory in its crude form has been entirely disproved. Nevertheless the controversy be- tween the evolutionists and epigenesists still exists in modified form. For it is evident that in the fertilized ovum there may exist preformed mechanisms or complexes of molecules which, while in no way resembling anatomically the subsequently developed parts of the organism, nevertheless are the foundation stones, to use a figure of speech, upon which the character of the adult structure depends. Such a view in one form or another is probably held by most bi- ologists, since it avoids the well-nigh inconceivable difficulties of- fered by a completely epigenetic theory. If the fertilized ovum of one animal is in the beginning substantially similar to that of any other animal the epigenesist must ascertain what combination of conditions during the process of development causes the egg, in a clog, for instance, to develop always into a dog, and moreover into a certain species of dog resembling more or less exactly the parent organisms. The infinite difficulties encountered by such a point of view are apparent.at once. In this, as in other similar prob- lems, experimental work is gradually accumulating facts which throw some light upon the matter and may eventually lead us to the right explanation. It has been made highly probable that the chro- matin material in the nuclei of the germ cells, the chromosomes, constitute the physical basis of hereditary transmission. In the fertilized egg, it will be remembered, half of the chromosomes come from the mother and half from the father, and there is good reason for believing that the maternal chromosomes are the bearers of the maternal characteristics, and the chromosomes derived from the spermatozoon convey the hereditary peculiarities of the father. Such a view, it will be noticed, implies at once preformed structures in the chromosomes and constitutes one form of an evolutionary hypothesis. This view is further supported by the interesting ex- periments of Wilson.* This author has shown that in certain molluscs (Dentalium or Patella) if a portion of the egg is cut off, the remaining portion upon fertilization develops into a defective animal that is not a whole embryo, but rather a piece or fragment of an embryo. Or if the fertilized egg after its first segmentation is separated artificially into two independent cells each develops an embryo, but neither one is completely formed, — each is lacking in certain structures and * Wilson, "Science," February 24, 1905, for a popular discussion; also "Journal of Experimental ZoSlogy," 1, 1 and 197, 1904, and 2, 371, 1905. HEREDITY. 955 the two must be taken together to constitute an entirely normal animal. By experiments of this kind it has been shown that cer- tain definite portions of the egg are responsible for the formation of particular organs in the adult. If these portions of the egg are removed the organs in question are not developed. Facts of this kind lead to the evolutionary view that in the fertilized ovum there is a collection of different materials designated as formative stuffs each of which is specific, — that is, develops into a special structure. Many facts connected with the regeneration of parts, — regeneration of a lost leg in a crab, for example — may be used to support a similar view of the existence of specific formative stuffs in the cells of the body.* Wilson has suggested an attractive theory which seems to account for the facts known at present and forms an acceptable com- promise between the extremes of epigenesis and evolution. Accord- ing to him, the germ (fertilized ovum) contains two elements, one of which undergoes a development that is essentially epigenetic, while the other contains a preformed structure which controls and deter- mines the course of development. The first is represented by the cytoplasm of the egg, the second by the chromatin (chromosomes) of the nucleus. The latter have specific structures, and under their influence the nutritive undifferentiated material of the cytoplasm is modified to form specific formative stuffs differing in character in the developing ova of different animals. Many interesting gen- eral theories of heredity have been proposed by Darwin, Nageli, Weissmann, Mendel, Galton, Brooks, and others. It is impossible to give here an outline of all these theories, but a word may be said regarding the work of cle Vries and Mendel, which have given rise recently to so much discussion. For fuller information the reader is referred to special treatises on the subject. f According to the well-known views of Darwin in regard to the action of natural selection it was assumed that new varieties and species are formed by the cumulative action of selection upon small fluctuating variations. By this cumulative selection certain variations are preserved and strengthened until thejr are suffi- ciently marked to constitute a specific difference, the process requiring naturally a long period of time. In contrast with this view de Vries has suggested what is commonly known as the theory of mutations. According to this view the variability in the germ plasm is such that it may at times give rise not to fluctu- ating variations but to marked and permanent variations, and * For a discussion of these facts and for various hypotheses, see Morgan, "Regeneration," New York, 1901. t Hertwig, "The Biological Problems of To-day"; Delage, "L'heredite- et les grands problemes de la biologic generale," 1903; Thomson, "Heredity," 1908; Kellogg, "Darwinism To-day," 1908; Jordan and Kellogg, "Evolution and Animal Life," 1907. 956 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. these latter, if advantageous to the animal, are preserved by natural selection. Such permanent variations are known as mutations or ''sports/' and in consequence of their formation and preservation the process of evolution may proceed much more rapidly than was assumed to be the case in the original form of Darwin's hypothesis. The contribution made to our understanding of heredity by the work of Mendel and those who have used his conceptions is most significant. By the Menclelian law or Menclelian inheritance is meant in the first place the general idea that characteristics handed down by inheritance from parents to offspring may be treated as separate units. In some cases parental characteristics may blend in the children, as for example, in the case of color, the mulatto being in this regard a blend of a white and a black parent. In other cases, however, there is no blending, but an alternation of one or the other of a pair of con- trasting characteristics. As regards such a pair of alternating characteristics Mendel found that one will be dominant, the other recessive, whenever they are brought together. That is to say, if each parent possesses one of such alternating characteristics, brown eyes and blue eyes, for example, the children will all show the dominant characteristic, in this case brown eyes, but the other characteristic will be present in a recessive or concealed form. In the hybrids possessing both characteristics the germ cells are so divided that half of them possess the dominant alone and half the recessive alone. This constitutes the law of the "purity of the germ cells" or of the " segregation of the gametes." If two such hybrids breed together it follows from the law of probabilities that in the offspring three out of four will show the dominant characteristic and one the recessive characteristic. Moreover, of those that show the dominant characteristic two will be hybrids, containing also the recessive, but one will be a pure dominant. This result may be understood from the following formula, in which D and R represent respectively the dominant and the recessive: D— Tt | | = 1 DD, 2D(R) and 1 RR. U— R If two pure recessives or two pure dominants breed together, only a recessive or a dominant, as the case may be, will be exhibited in the offspring, and in this way pure characteristics may be selected and established. Such a process of selection is simple in the case of the recessive characteristics; but in the case of the dominant it is, of course, more difficult to distinguish between the \)\) and the D(R). The distinction may be made by breeding DETERMINATION OF SEX. 957 with an animal showing the recessive. If the dominant is pure, all of the offspring will exhibit the dominant characteristics. If, on the contrary, it is a hybrid, the offspring will be half dominant and half recessive, according to the formula: D— R | |=DR, DR, RR, RR. R— R The many attempts to verify this law in breeding have shown that it expresses probably a great truth, although the application of it to the practical purposes of breeding is beset with many compli- cations. In regard to mankind the chief lesson that may be learned from these investigations is the very great importance of hereditary transmission — characteristics, good or bad, that dis- appear in one generation may simply be recessive, and in the course of successive intermarriages may again come to the surface in accordance with the Mendelian law. Determination of Sex. — The conditions which lead to the determination of the sex of the developing ovum have attracted much investigation and speculation. In the absence of precise data very numerous and oftentimes very peculiar theories have been advanced.* Such views as the following have been main- tained: that the sex is determined by the ova alone; that it is determined by the spermatozoa alone; that one side (right ovary or testis) contains male elements, the other female; that the sex is a result of the interaction of the ovum and spermatozoon, the most virile element producing its own sex, or according to another possibility "the superior parent produces the opposite sex"; that the sex depends on the time relation of coitus to menstruation, fertilization before menstruation favoring male births, after men- struation female births; that it depends upon the nutritive con- ditions of the ovum during development or of the maternal parent ; that it depends upon the relative ages of the parents; that there are preformed male and female ova and male and female sper- matozoa, etc. What we may call the scientific study of the problem began with the collection of statistics of births. Statistics in Europe of 5,935,000 births indicate that 106 male children are born to 100 female, and the data from other countries show the same fact of an excess of male children. It may be added as a matter of interest that this preponderance of the male is reversed at the period of. adult life. Owing to the greater death rate of the male the proportion of male to female in the adult population of Europe * For accounts of the various theories and discussion, see Morgan, "Popular Science Monthly," December, 1903, and "Experimental Zoology," 1907; Len- hossek, "Das Problem der geschlechtsbestimmenden Ursachen," 1903. 958 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. is as 1000 to 1024. Examination of these statistics with reference to determining conditions led to the formulation of the so-called Hof acker-Sadler law or laws, which may be stated as follows: (1) When the man is older than the woman the ratio of male births is increased (113 to 100). (2) When the parents are of equal age the ratio of female births is increased (93.5 males to 100 females). (3) When the woman is older the ratio of female births is still further increased (88.2 to 100). These laws have been corroborated by some statisticians and contradicted or modi- fied bjr others. Ploss attempted to show that poor nutritive con- ditions affecting the parents, especially the mother, favor the birth of boys. Dusing combined these results in a sort of general compensatory law of nature, according to which a deficiency in either sex leads, by a process of natural selection, to an increase in the births of the opposite sex. Thus, when males are few in number, — as the result, for instance, of wars, — females marry later and more males are produced. When males are in excess early marriages are the rule and this condition favors an excess of female births. However interesting these statistics may be, it is very evident that they do not touch the real problem of the cause of the determination of sex. Modern work has turned largely to observations and direct experiments upon the lower animals, particularly the inverte- brates, with the result that a very large number of facts have been collected of a most interesting kind, but difficult as yet to interpret so as to formulate a general law. The trend of modern work tends to oppose an older view founded largely upon experi- ments on frogs, bees, and wasps, according to which the sex is not determined at or before fertilization, but is controlled or may be controlled by the conditions of nourishment during development, favorable conditions of nutriment leading to the development of female cells from the germinal epithelium of the embryo. In contrast with this latter view an opinion that has been frequently advocated is that the sex of the embryo is determined in the egg before fertilization or at the time of fertilization. This view, as first presented, assumed substantially that there are male and female eggs to begin with, and that the determination of sex resides in the maternal organism alone. Some of the facts that support this view with more or less conclusiveness are as follows: (1) In certain worms (Dinophilus) eggs of two sizes are produced; the large eggs on fertilization develop always into females, the small ones into males. Similar facts are recorded for other animals (Hydatina). (2) Many species of invertebrates exhibit the phenomenon of parthenogenesis — that is, the eggs of the mother develop without fertilization. In some cases this method DETERMINATION OF SEX. 959 forms the only means of reproduction, and the individuals of the race are all females. But in other animals reproduction is effected either by parthenogenesis or by fertilization, according to the conditions — change of seasons, etc. Among these latter animals it may be shown, in some cases at least, that the parthenogenetic eggs may give rise either to males or females — a fact which accords with the hypothesis of the existence of male and female eggs in the mother. (3) In man twins may be born and these twins may be of two kinds. First, those that are developed from two different eggs, each of which has its own chorion and develops its own placenta. This kind may be designated as false twins, and in the matter of sex they may be male and female, or both male, or both female. The matter varies as in the statis- tics of births in general. In the other group, however, of true twins or identical twins, the two embryos are developed from a single ovum and are included in a single chorion. In such cases the sexes of the twins are always the same, they are both boys ^or both girls. This fact favors the view that the sex may be pre- determined in the ovum, which may be either male or female. However, if we grant the fundamental fact, so far as the ova are concerned, that they are either male or female at the time of forma- tion or are made so during the process of growth and maturation, it is still logically possible that there may also be male and female spermatozoa, and that in the union of the two cells the sex of the fertilized ovum may be referable either to the ovum or spermatozoon. It is not justifiable to assert that the paternal organism is without influence upon the sex of the offspring. In fact, in the case of honey bees it is observed that if the egg of the queen bee is unfer- tilized it develops into a male, but, if fertilized, into a female, thus indicating a determining influence upon the part of the male ele- ment. Moreover, Wilson* and others have obtained some inter- esting results upon insects (hemiptera), which indicate that the determination of sex in these animals is dependent upon or correlated with a visible difference in the chromosomes of the spermatozoon. In some cases one-half of the spermatozoa contain an unpaired or accessor}^ chromosome. Those showing this structure produce females on fertilization, while the others give rise to males. In other cases all the spermatozoa exhibit the same number of chromosomes, but one-half of them contain a large " idiochromosome, " the other half a small one. The former give rise on fertilization to females, the latter to males. If, on the basis of such facts, we assume the existence of male and female eggs- and male and female spermatozoa, the question of the sex of the offspring would seem to depend upon which of * Wilson, "The Journal of Experimental Zoology, " 1906, hi., 1. 960 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. the sex-determining structures in the two cells predominates after union. While it is difficult or impossible to make any generalizations in regard to sex determination which will hold throughout the animal kingdom, or which convey any definite suggestion of an explanation of the problem, two facts of impor- tance may be especially emphasized: First, The capacity to produce a male or a female is probably contained in both the eggs and the spermatozoa, or, to quote Morgan's words, "In all species with separate sexes the potentialities of producing both sexes are present in all eggs and in all sperm, but the development of the one or the other sex is determined by some unknown internal relation." Second, In those cases in which an individual de- velops from a fertilized ovum, the sex is probably determined at the time of fertilization. Growth and Senescence. — The body increases rapidly after birth in size and weight. It is the popular idea that the rate of growth increases up to maturity and then declines as old age ad- vances. As a matter of fact, careful examination of the facts shows that the rate of growth decreases from birth to old age, although not uniformly. At the pubertal period and at other times its downward tendency may be arrested for a time. But, speaking generally, the maximum rate of growth is reached some time during the intra- uterine period, and after birth the curve falls steadily. Senescence has begun to appear at the time we are born.* Thus, according to the statistics of Quetelet, the average male child weighs at birth 6^- pounds. At the end of the first year it weighs 18^ pounds, a gain of 12 pounds. At the end of the second year it weighs 23 pounds, a gain of only 4£ pounds, and so on, the rate of increase falling rap- idly with advancing years. Jacksonf has published an interesting series of observations upon the relative and absolute growth of the human fetus and its different organs during the intra-uterine period. Relative growth is defined as the "ratio of the gain during a given period to the weight at the beginning of the period." from this standpoint he finds that the maximum rate of growth occurs during the first month of fetal life. As determined by the volume of the fetus the ovum increases more than 10,000 times in size; during this period. In the succeeding months of intra-uterine life the relative monthly growth rate may be expressed by i he figures 74, 1 1 , 1 .75, .82, .67, .50, .47, .45. During this period the absolute weight is, of course, increasing rapidly, and according to Jackson's observations the total weight of the embryo may be (A °*o fd*ivs>)\4 7 ) ♦ See Minot, "Journal of Physiology," 12, 07. f Jackson, "The American journal of Anatomy," 9, I I'.), L909. GROWTH AXD SEXESCEXCE. 961 The actual statistics of growth have been collected and tabulated with great care by a number of observers; for this country especially by Bowditch, Porter, and Beyer.* An interesting feature of the records collected by Bowditch is the proof that the prepubertal acceleration of growth comes earlier in girls than in boys, so that between the ages of twelve and fifteen the average girl is heavier and taller than the boy. Later, the boy's growth is accelerated and his stature and weight increase beyond that of the girl. It appears from the examinations made upon school children by Porter and by Beyer that a high degree of physical development is usually associated with a corresponding pre-eminence in mental ability. The signs of old age may be de- tected in other ways than by observations upon the rate of growth. Changes take place in the composition of the tissues ; these changes, at first scarcely noticeable, become gradually more obvious as old age advances. The bones become more brittle from an increase in their inorganic salts, the cartilages become more rigid and calca- reous, the crystalline lens gradually loses its elasticity, the muscles lose their vigor, the hairs their pigment, the nuclei of the nerve cells become smaller, and so on. In every way there is increasing evidence, as the years grow, that the metabolism of the living mat- ter of the body becomes less and less perfect ; the power of the protoplasm itself becomes more and more limited, and we may suppose would eventually fail, bringing about what might be called a natural death. As a matter of fact, death of the organism usually results from the failure of some one of its many complex mechanisms, while the majority of the tissues are still able to maintain their exis- tence if supplied with proper conditions of nourishment. The phys- iological evidences of an increasing senescence warrant the view, however, that death is a necessary result of the properties of living matter in all the tissues except possibl}T the reproductive elements. The course of metabolism is such that it is self-limited, and even if perfect conditions were supplied natural death would eventually result. We do not understand the nature of these limitations, — that is, the ultimate causes of senescence. Many examples of unusual longevity are on record, the most authentic being probably that of Thomas Parr. An account of his life and the results of a postmor- tem examination by Harvey are given in volume iii of the " Philo- sophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London." "He died after he had outlived nine princes, in the tenth year of the tenth of them, at the age of one hundred and fifty- two years and nine months." The immediate cause of his death was attributed to a * See Bowditch, "Report of State Board of Health of Massachusetts," 1877, 1879, and 1891; Porter, "Transactions, Academy of Science," St. Louis, 1893-94; Beyer, "Proceedings, United States Naval Institute," 21, 297, 1895. 61 962 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. change of food and air and habits of life, as he was brought from Shrop- shire to London, "where he fed high and drunk plentifully of the best wines."* With reference to the phenomenon of senescense as a neces- sary attribute of living matter, Weissmann has called attention to the fact that inasmuch as the species continues to exist after the in- dividual dies, we must believe that the protoplasm of the repro- ductive elements is not subject to natural death, but has a self- perpetuating metabolism which under proper conditions makes it immortal. Weissmann f designates the protoplasm of the germ cells as germ-plasm, that of the rest of the body as somatoplasm, and inasmuch as the former continues to propagate itself indefinite^ under proper conditions, while the latter has a limited existence, he concludes that originally protoplasm possessed the property of potential immortality. That is, barring accidents, disease, etc., it was capable of reproducing itself indefinitely. He assumes, more- over, that this property is exhibited at present in many of the sim- pler forms of life, such as the ameba. This latter phase of his theory has been the subject of much interesting investigation,! but con- clusive results have not been reached. Assuming that the poten- tial immortality exhibited by the reproductive cells was originally a general property of protoplasm, Weissman conceives that the phe- nomenon of senescence and death exhibited by other cells, somato- plasm, is a secondary property, which was acquired as a result of variation and was preserved by natural selection because it is an advantage in the propagation of the species. An actual immor- tality of the entire organism, — that is, the property of indefinite existence except as death might be caused by accidental occur- rences of various kinds — would be a disadvantage in many ways. The vast increase in the number of individuals might exceed the capacity of nature to provide for; the retention of the maimed and imperfect would make many useless mouths to feed, and perhaps the evolution of higher and more perfect forms by the slow action of variation and natural selection would be retarded. From this point of view senility and natural death constitute a beneficial adaptation, acquired because of its utility to the race, on the one hand, and, on the other, because, after the beginning of a differen- tiation in function among the cells, the possession of immortality by all the cells was no longer of any value to the race, and therefore was not brought under the preserving influence of natural selection. * A picture of Parr painted by van Dyck (i635) is exhibited in the lioya' Gallery, Dresden, No. L032. f W< i man, "Essays upon Heredity and Kindred Biological Prob- lems"; al o " Germ-plasm" in the "Contemporary Scieiri- Series. J See Maupas, Archives de zodlogie exp6rimentale et gen6rale," (i, 165, 1888; Joukowsky, "Inaugural Dissertation," Heidelberg, ]898; Gotte, "Ueber den Ursprung des Todes," 1883. GROWTH AND SENESCENCE. 963 Perhaps the most significant and definite contribution to the subject of growth has been made by Rubner* upon the basis of the energy factor. His estimates were made upon data collected for man and the following mammalia, horse, cow, sheep, pig, dog, cat, rabbit, and guinea-pig — and they bring out the sur- prising fact that human growth constitutes a type of its own differing greatly from that shown by the other mammals named. His conclusions are expressed in two general laws which are founded upon calculations made upon these animals in the first period after birth during the time necessary for doubling the weight of the animal: First, the law of constant energy consumption. During the first period of growth the total amount of energy necessary for maintenance (metabolism) and growth, as expressed by the heat value of the food consumed, is the same for all mammals except man. To form one kilogram of animal weight requires in round numbers 4808 calories in food; while for man about six times this amount is needed. Since the several mammals con- sidered require very different times to double their weight, it follows from this law that the shorter the time necessary for this result the more intense Avill be the metabolism, or, expressed in another way, the rapidity of growth is proportional to the intensity of the metabolic processes. Second, the law of the constant growth quotient. In all the mammals considered, with the exception of man, the same fractional part of the entire food energy is utilized for growth. This fractional portion is designated as the "growth- quotient," and it averages 34 per cent., that is to say, for every 1000 calories of food 340 calories are applied to growth. In man, on the contrary, the growth quotient is only 5 per cent. This growth quotient is a specific property of the cell and a charac- teristic of youthful ness. It has its maximal value at birth, so far as extra-uterine life is concerned, and then sinks slowly, so that at maturity, that is, at the end of the growth period, it becomes zero. Thence forward the energy of the food is utilized only for the maintenance of the cells and for the work they perform, none is applied to growth. Rubner suggests that the power to grow possessed by the cells of the young organism depends upon some special mechanisms of a chemical nature, that is, probably certain special chemical complexes which he designates as the "growth mechanism " (Wachstumstrieb) . After the period of maturity has been reached the question arises whether the subsequent duration of life can be foretold or formulated in any definite way. The older naturalists conceived that the duration of mature life might represent a definite multiple of the period of youth. According to Buffon this multiple is 6 to * Rubner, "Das Problem der Lebensdauer, " etc., Berlin, 1908. 964 THE PHYSIOLOGY OF REPRODUCTION. 7, according to Flourens it is 5 — that is, the mean duration of life is 5 to 7 times that required for the completion of growth. The data gathered in regard to the average duration of life among different animals has not borne out these suggestions, and Rubner discusses the matter again from the energy standpoint. He estimates the number of calories of food which are required for each kilogram of body weight in the different mammalia from the end of the period of youth to the end of life. For man this period is estimated at sixty years (20 to 80). On this basis he finds that each human kilogram requires 725,770 calories, while for the other mammalia for which data are accessible an average of only 191,600 calories is required, and the figures in the latter animals are so close as almost to warrant the belief that the same amount is required by each animal in spite of the great variation in the duration of life. It follows from these figures that the human cell is characterized, as compared with that of the other mammalia, by its much greater total capacity for obtaining energy from the foodstuffs. This capacity, the property of assimilation, implies chemical changes and transformations in the living matter, and the fact that eventually this property languishes and expires, that is, the fact that there is such a thing as natural or physiological death, means that the somatic protoplasm is capable of effecting only a limited number of such transformations. In man a greater number is possible than in the other mammals, and among the latter the number is practically the same, but in the smaller animals, with their more intense metabolism, the series is com- pleted in a shorter time than in the case of the larger animals. Rubner states, moreover, that if a cell, the yeast cell, for example, by artificial means is forced to live without growing and multiply- ing it dies in a very short time. In some way the processes of growth contain the very source of the maintenance of life. The injurious by-products which accompany simple metabolism in the living matter are in some way obviated or neutralized by the growth changes. On this basis Rubner suggests, somewhat in the line of Darwin's theory of pangenesis and of Weissmann's theory of the cause of death in the somatoplasm, that the body- cells give off certain molecular complexes which are necessary to the growth processes, and these complexes are taken up by the reproductive cells. After the animal has reached the period of puberty, of reproductive power, and provision is thus made for the perpetuation of the species, the individual organism is depleted of the power of growth, and senescence and death become inevitable. APPENDIX. PROTEINS AND THEIR CLASSIFICATION. Definition and General Structure. — Proteins or albumins are complex organic compounds containing nitrogen which, although differing much in their composition, are related in their properties. They are formed by living matter, and occur in the tissues and liquids of plants and animals, of which they form the most characteristic constituent. On ultimate analy- sis they are all found to contain carbon, hydrogen, oxygen, and nitrogen; most of them contain also some sulphur, and some, in addition, phosphorus or iron. As usually obtained, they leave also some ash when incinerated, showing that they hold in combination some inorganic salts. Percentage analyses of the most common proteins of the body show that the above named constituents occur in the following proportions: Carbon 50 to 55 per cent. Hydrogen 6.5 to 7.3 " " Nitrogen 15 to 17.6 " " Oxygen 19 to 24 " Sulphur 0.3 to 2.4 " The clearest insight into the structure of the protein molecule has been obtained by a study of its decomposition products. When submitted to the action of proteolytic enzymes, or putrefaction, or acid at high temperatures, the large molecules split into a number of simpler bodies in consequence of hydrolytic cleavage. These end-products are very numerous, and, while they differ somewhat for the different proteins, yet a number of them are the same or similar for all proteins. The great variety in the end-products is an indication of the complexity of the molecule, while their similarity is proof that the various proteins are all built, so to speak, upon a common plan, by the union of certain groupings which may be more numerous in one protein than in another. This fact becomes evident from a brief consideration of the prod- ucts obtained by hydrolytic cleavage with acids. The groupings represented by the following compounds may be supposed to exist preformed in protein molecules, some possibly containing them all, some only a portion of the list, while the different groups vary in their proportional amounts in the various proteins: Monamino Acids. 1. Glycocoll or glycin (amino-ace-tic acid). 2. Alanin (aminopropionic acid). 3. Valin (amino valerianic acid). 4. Leucin (aminocaproic acid). 5. Isoleucin (aminocaproic acid). 6. Serin (oxaminopropionic acid). 7. Cystein (aminothiopropionic acid). 8. Phenylalanin (phenylaminopropionic acid). 9. Tyrosin ioxy phenylaminopropionic acid). 10. Tryptophan (indolaminopropionic acid). 11. Aspartic acid (aminosuccinic acid).. 12. Glutaminic acid (aminoglutaric acid). 13. Prolin (pyrrolidin-carboxyhc acid). 14. Oxyprolin (oxypyrrolidin-carboxylic acid). 15. Histidin (imidazol aminopropionic acid). Diamino Acids. 16. Lysin (diaminocaproic acid). 17. Arginin iguanidinaminovalerianic acid). IS. Diaminotrioxydodecoic acid. 965 966 APPENDIX. These split products are all amino-acids, some of them belonging to the fatty acid (aliphatic) series of carbon compounds, some to the aromatic (carbocyclic) series, and some to the heterocyclic (pyrrol, indol) series. In what may be considered the simplest proteins occurring in nature — namely, the protamins found in the spermatozoa — only from four to six of these groups occur, while in some of the more familiar proteins, such as serum-albumin or casein, a much larger number is found. This fact is illustrated by the following table, taken from Abderhalden, which shows the composition of several proteins belonging to different classes. It will be noted that except for the salmin the known products sum up to less than 100 per cent., showing that there is a large portion of the molecule as yet unknown. Serum Serum Casein. Salmin. Albumin. Globulin. Glycin 0 3.5 0 Alanin 2.7 2.2 0.9 Valin present 1.0 4.3 Leucin 20.0 18.7 10.5 Prolin 1.0 2.8 3.1 11.0 Phenylalanine 3.1 3.8 3.2 Glutaminic acid 7.7 8.5 11.0 Aspartic acid 3.1 2.5 1.2 Cystin 2.3 0.7 .065. Serin 0.6 .... 0.23 7.8 Tyrosin 2.1 2.5 4.5 Tryptophan present present 1.5 Diaminotrioxydodecoic acid .... 0.75' Oxyprolin .... 0.25 Lysin .... 5.80 Arginin 4.84 87.4 Histidin 2.59 The «-amino-acids of which these end-products consist all contain the H grouping — C— NH2, and Fischer has shown that such bodies possess the COOH property of combining with one another to make complex molecules containing two, three, or more groups of amino-acids. The combination takes place with the elimination of water formed by the union of the OH of the carboxyl (COOH) group in one acid and the H of the amino (NH2) group in another. Thus, two molecules of amino-acetic acid (glycocoll) may be made to unite to form a compound, glycylglycin, as follows: NH2CH2COOH + NH2CH2COOH — H20 = NH2CH2CONHCH2COOH. Glycocoll. Glycocoll. Glycylglycin. Compounds <»f this kind are designated by Fischer as peptids. _ When formed from the union of two amino-acids they are known as dipeptids; from three, as tripeptids, etc. The more complicate;! compounds of this sort, the poly- peptide, begin to show reactions similar to those of the proteins. Sonic of them give tin; biuret reaction, some are acted upon and split by proteolytic enzymes. It seems justifiable, therefore, to consider proteins as essentially polypeptid compounds of greater or less complexity— that is, they are acid- amids formed by the union of a number of a-amino-acid compounds. More than ;i hundred* of these artificial polypeptids have been thus synthesized, one of the most complex, an octa-deca peptid, consisting of eighteen mon- amino acids, fifteen molecules of glycin, and three of leucin, with a total molec- ular weight of 1213. This conception of the structure of the protein molecule explains a number of their general characteristics — for instance: (I) The fad i h.'t I hey are nil decomposed and yield similar products under the influence PROTEINS AND THEIR CLASSIFICATION. 967 of proteolytic enzymes or boiling dilute acid. (2) The fact that the proteins are all so alike in their general properties in spite of the great differences in the complexity of their molecular structure. (3) The fact that they show both basic and acid characters. (4) The fact that they all give the biuret reaction* (see below). In addition to the amino-acids some proteins — egg-albumin, for example — yield a carbohydrate body upon decomposition. The carbohydrate ob- tained is an amino-sugar compound, usually glucosamin, C6H]3N05. It is detected by its reducing action and by the formation of an osazone. It seems probable, therefore, that some of the proteins at least contain such a group- ing as part of the molecular complex, but at present it is undetermined how many possess this peculiarity of structure. General Reactions of the Proteins. — It is evident from what has been said in the preceding paragraph that proteins may give different reactions according to the kinds of groupings contained in the molecule. The reac- tions common to all proteins are few in number, the most certain perhaps being the biuret reaction, the hydrolysis by proteolytic enzymes or putre- factive organisms, and the nature of the split products formed by these latter hydrolyses or by the action of boiling dilute acids. A very large number of reactions, however, have been described which hold for some or all of the proteins usually found in the tissues and liquids of the body. These reactions may be described under two heads: (1) Precipitation of the protein when in solution; (2) color reactions. /. Precipitants. — For one or another protein the following reagents cause precipitation : 1. The addition of an excess of alcohol. 2. Boiling (heat coagulation). 3. The addition of mineral acids, — e. g., nitric acid. 4. The salts of the heavy metals, — e. g., acetate of lead, copper sul- phate, etc. 5. Addition of neutral salts of the alkalies to a greater or less degree of concentration, — e. g., sodium chlorid, ammonium sulphate. 6. Ferrocyanid of potassium after previous acidification by acetic acid. 7. Tannic acid after previous acidification by acetic acid. 8. Phosphotungstic or phosphomolybdic acid in the presence of free mineral acids. 9 Iodin in solution in potassium iodid, after previous acidification with a mineral acid. 10. Picric acid in solutions acidified by organic acids. 11. Trichloracetic acid. This list might be extended still further, but it comprises the precipi- tating reagents that are ordinarily used. Some of them, particularly Nos. 7, 8, and 9, give reactions in solutions containing excessively minute traces of protein. 12. Precipitins. In this connection a brief reference may be made to the interesting group of bodies known as precipitins. As stated on p. 412, the animal organism has the power, when foreign cells are injected into it, of forming anti-bodies by a specific biological reaction. It has been discovered that anti-bodies, or as they are called in this case, precipitins, may be produced in the same, way if protein solutions or solutions of animal tissue are in- jected into the circulation. Thus, if cows' milk be injected under the skin of a rabbit there will be produced within the rabbit's blood a precipitin which is capable of precipitating the casein of cows' milk, although it may have no action on the milk of other animals. In the same way any given foreign protein, when injected under the skin of an animal, may cause the production of a pre- * For further details, see Cohnheim, "Chemie der Eiweisskorper, " second 'edition, 1904; or Abderhalden, "Physiological Chemistry," translated by Hall and Defren, New York, 1908, and Rosenheim, in "Science Progress," April and July, 1908. 968 APPENDIX. cipitin capable of precipitating that particular protein from its solutions. The precipitin is not absolutely specific for the protein used to produce it, but nearly so. If a rabbit is immunized with human blood a precipitin is produced in the animal's blood which causes a precipitate when mixed with human blood or with that of some of the higher monkeys, but gives no reaction with the blood of other mammals. The reaction may be used, therefore, in a measure to test the blood-relationship of different animals.* It has been suggested that the reaction may also be of practical importance in medicolegal cases, in determining whether a given blood-stain is or is not human blood. For such a pur- pose a human antiserum is first produced by injecting human serum into a rabbit. The serum of the rabbit is then mixed with an extract of the suspected blood-stain made with salt solution; if a precipitate forms it proves that the blood stain is human blood provided the possibility of its being monkey's blood is excluded. Concerning the nature of the precipitins, little is known. They combine quantitatively with the protein precipitated and they are inactivated (hematosera) by a temperature of 70° C. Their reactions are not sufficiently specific to be used as a means of de- tecting or distinguishing closely related proteins. //. The Color Reactions of Proteins. 1. The biuret reaction. The protein solution is made strongly alkaline with caustic socla or potash and a few drops of a dilute solution of copper sulphate are added carefully so as to avoid an excess. A purple color is obtained. Some proteins (peptones) give a red purple, others a blue purple. If only a blue color, without any mixture of red, is obtained, no protein is present. At present this reaction gives the best single test for protein. It obtains its name from the fact that it is given by biuret HN., LL.D., F. R. C. S. (Hon.), Professor of the Principles of Surgery and of Clin- ical Surgery, Jefferson Medical College, Philadelphia. Octavo volume of 441 pages, illustrated. Cloth, $3.75 net, Keen on the Surgery of Typhoid The Surgical Complications and Sequels of Typhoid Fever. By Wi. W. Keen, M.D., LL.D., F.R.C.S. (Hon.), Professor of the Principles of Surgery and of Clinical Surgery, Jefferson Medical College, Philadelphia, etc Octavo volume of 386 pages, illustrated. Cloth, $3.00 net. ** Every surgical incident which can occur during or after typhoid fever is amply discussed and fully illustrated by cases. . . . The book will be useful both to the surgeon and physician." — 'J he Practitioner, London. Lewis' Anatomy and Physiology for Nurses Just Ready— New (2d) Edition Anatomy and Physiology for Nurses. By LeRoy Lewis, M. D., Surgeon to and Lecturer on Anatomy and Physiology for Nurses at the Lewis Hospital, Bay City, Michigan. i2mo, 375 pages, with 150 illustrations. Cloth, $1.75 net. A demand for such a work as this, treating the subjects from the nurse's point of view, has long existed. Dr. Lewis has based the plan and scope of this work on the methods em- ployed by him in teaching these branches, making the text unusually simple and clear. " It is not in any sense rudimentary, but comprehensive in its treatment of the subjects in hand." — Nurses, Journal of the Pacific Coast. McClellan's Art Anatomy Anatomy in Its Relation to Art. By George McClellan, M. D. , Professor of Anatomy, Pennsylvania Academy of the Fine Arts. Quarto volume, 9 by \iyi inches, with 338 original drawings and photographs, and 260 pages of text. Dark blue vellum, $10.00 net ; Half Russia, $12.50 net. 14 SAUNDERS* BOOKS ON Haynes' Anatomy A Manual of Anatomy. By Irving S. Haynes, M.D., Professor of Prac- tical Anatomy, Cornell University Medical College. Octavo, 680 pages, with 42 diagrams and 134 full-page half-tones. Cloth, $2.50 net. American Pocket Dictionary ^SSiSSL The American Pocket Medical Dictionary. Edited by W. A. Newman Dorland, A.M..M.D., Assistant Obstetrician, Hospital of the University of Pennsylvania, etc. 598 pages. Full leather, limp, with gold edges, $1.00 net; with patent thumb index, $1.25 net. Grant on Face, Mouth, and Jaws A Text*- Book of the Surgical Principles and Surgical Diseases of the Face, Mouth, and Jaws. For Dental Students. By H. Horace Grant, A.M., M.D., Professor of Surgery and of Clinical Surgery, Hospital College of Medicine. Octavo of 231 pages, with 68 illustrations. Cloth, $2.50 net. Fowler's Surgery In Two Volumes A Treatise on Surgery. By George R. Fowler, M. D., Emeritus Pro- fessor of Surgery, New York Polyclinic. Two imperial octavos of 725 pages each, with 888 original text-illustrations and 4 colored plates. Per set : Cloth, $15.00 net ; Half Morocco, $18.00 net. International Text- Book of Surgery second Edition The International Text=Book of Surgery. In two volumes. By Ameri- can and British authors. Edited by J. Collins Warren, M. D., LL. D., F. R. C. S. (Hon.), Professor of Surgery, Harvard Medical School ; and A. Pearce Gould, M. S., F. R. C. S. , of London, England. Vol. I. : General and Operative Surgery. Royal octavo, 975 pages, 461 illustrations, 9 full- page colored plates. Vol. II. : Special or Regional Surgery. Royal octavo, 1 1 22 pages, 499 illustrations, and 8 full-page colored plates. Per volume : Cloth, $5.00 net ; Half Morocco, $6.50 net. American Text-Book of Surgery Fourth Edition American Text-Book of Surgery. Edited by W. W. Keen, M. D., LL. D., Hon. F. R. C. S., Eng. and Edin., and J. William White, \1. D., Ph. D. Octavo, 1363 pages, 551 text-cuts and 39 colored and half-tone plates. Cloth, #7.00 net ; Half Morocco, #8.50 net. Robson and Cammidge on the Pancreas The Pancreas : Its Surgery and Pathology. By A. W. Mayo Robson, I •'. R. C. S., of London, England; and P. J. Cammidge, F. R. C. S., of London, England. Octavo of 546 pages, illustrated. Cloth, $5.00 net; Half Morocco, #6.50 net. SURGERY AND ANATOMY. 15 American Illustrated Dictionary ^ New ,,£'£ The American Illustrated Medical Dictionary. With tables of Arteries, Muscles, Nerves, Veins, etc. ; of Bacilli, Bacteria, etc. ; Eponymic Tables of Diseases, Operations, Stains, Tests, etc. By W. A. Newman Dorland, M.D. Large octavo, 876 pages. Flexible leather, $4.50 net; with thumb index, $5.00 net. Howard A. Kelly, M.D., Professor of Gynecology \ Johns Hopkins University , Baltimore. "Dr. Dorland's dictionary is admirable. It is so well gotten up and of such con- venient size. No errors have been found in my use of it." Golebiewski and Bailey's Accident Diseases Atlas and Epitome of Diseases Caused by Accidents. By Dr. Ed. Golebiewski, of Berlin. Edited, with additions, by Pearce Bailey, M.D. Consulting Neurologist to St. Luke's Hospital, New York City. With 71 colored figures on 40 plates, 143 text-cuts, and 549 pages of text. Cloth, $4.00 net. In Saunders'' Hand- Atlas Series. Helferich and Bloodgood on Fractures Atlas and Epitome of Traumatic Fractures and Dislocations By Prof. Dr. H. Helferich, of Greifswald, Prussia. Edited, with ad- ditions, by Joseph C. Bloodgood, M. D., Associate in Surgery, Johns Hopkins University, Baltimore. 216 colored figures on 64 lithographic plates, 190 text-cuts, and 353 pages of text. Cloth, $3.00 net. In Saun- ders' Atlas Series. Sultan and Coley on Abdominal Hernias Atlas and Epitome of Abdominal Hernias. By Pr. Dr. G. Sul- tan, ofGottingen. Edited, with additions, by Wm. B. Coley, M. D., Clinical Lecturer and Instructor in Surgery, Columbia University, New York. 119 illustrations, 36 in colors, and 277 pages of text. Cloth, $3.00 net. In Saunders' Hand- Atlas Series. Warren's Surgical Pathology |ed^ Surgical Pathology and Therapeutics. By J. Collins Warren, M.D., LL.D., F.R.C.S. (Hon.), Professor of Surgery, Harvard Medical School. Octavo, 873 pages; 136 illustrations, t,^ in colors. Cloth, $5.00 net; Half Morocco, $6.50 net. Zuckerkandl and DaCosta's Surgery f3g£j| Atlas and Epitome of Operative Surgery. By Dr. O. Zucker- kandl, of Vienna. Edited, with additions, by J. Chalmers DaCosta, M. D., Professor of Surgery and Clinical Surgery, Jefferson Medical Col- lege, Phila. 40 colored plates, 278 text-cuts, and 410 pages of text. Cloth, $3.50 net. In Saunders' Atlas Series. 16 SURGER Y AND ANA TOMY Moore's Orthopedic Surgery A Manual of Orthopedic Surgery. By James E. Moore, M.D., Professor of Clinical Surgery, University of Minnesota, College of Medicine and Surgery. Octavo of 356 pages, handsomely illustrated. Cloth, $2.50 net. "The book is eminently practical. It is a safe guide in the understanding- and treatment of orthopedic cases. Should be owned by every surgeon and practitioner." — Annals of Surgery. Fowler's Operating Room New (2d) Edition The Operating Room and the Patient, By Russell S. Fowler, M. D., Surgeon to the German Hospital, Brooklyn, New York. Octavo of 284 pages, illustrated. Cloth, $2.00 net. Dr. Fowler has written his book for surgeons, nurses assisting at an operation, internes, and all others whose duties bring them into the operating room. It contains explicit directions for the preparation of material, instruments needed, position of patient, etc., all beautifully illustrated. Nancrede's Principles of Surgery New (2d) Edition Lectures on the Principles of Surgery. By Chas. B. Naxcrede, M.D., LL. D., Professor of Surgery and of Clinical Surgery, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor. Octavo, 407 pages, illustrated. Cloth, #2.50 net. " We can strongly recommend this book to all students and those who would see something of the scientific foundation upon which the art of surgery is built." — Quarterly Medical Journal, Sheffield, England. Nancrede's Essentials of Anatomy. Seventh Edition Essentials of Anatomy,' including the Anatomy of the Viscera. By Chas. B. Nancrede, M.D., Professor of Surgery and of Clinical Surgery, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor. Crown octavo, 388 pages ; 180 cuts. With an Appendix containing over 60 illustrations of the osteology of the body. Based on Gray 's Anatomy. Cloth, $1.00 net. In Saunders1 Question Compends. " The questions have been wisely selected, and the answers accurately and concisely given."— University Medical Magazine. Martin's Essentials of Surgery. Seve£evi?edtion Essentials of Surgery. Containing also Venereal Diseases, Surgical Land- marks, Minor and Operative Surgery, and a complete description, with illus- trations, of the Handkerchief and Roller Bandages. By Edward Martin, A.M., M.D., Professor of Clinical Surgery, University of Pennsylvania, etc. Crown octavo, 338 pages, illustrated, With an Appendix on Antiseptic Sur- gery, etc. Cloth, jUi.on net. In Saunders1 Question Compends. " Written to assist the student, it will be A undoubted value to the practitioner, containing as it doi the essence of surgical work." — Boston Medical and Surgical Journal. Martin's Essentials of Minor Surgery, Band- aging, and Venereal Diseases. Seco£di^onVised Essentials of Minor Surgery, Bandaging, and Venereal Diseases. By EDWARD MARTIN, A.M., M.D., Professor of Clinical Surgery, University of Pennsylvania, etc. Crown octavo, 166 pages, with 78 illustrations. Cloth, $1.00 net. In Saunders1 Question Compends. "Tli* besl condensation of the subjects of which it treats yet placed before the profession,"— The Medical News, Philadelphia, COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY LIBRARIES This book is due on the date indicated below, or at the expiration of a definite period after the date of borrowing, as provided by the rules of the Library or by special arrange- ment with the Librarian in charge. DATE BORROWED DATE BORROWED DATE DUE C28(ll4l)M100 COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY LIBRARIES (hsl.stx) QP 34 H831 1909 C.1 A text-book of phvsioloqyfonriedica^t aF34- ***** V\o«HL \\ m * i