Skip to main content

Full text of "Aggression and familiarity as factors in mate selection in Peromyscus polionotus and Peromyscus maniculatus"

See other formats


AGGRESSION  AMD  FAMILIARITY  AS  FACTORS  IN 

MATE  SELECTION  IN  Peromyscus  pol ionotus 

AND  Peromyscus  maniculatus 


BY 
DANIEL  GEORGE  WEBSTER 


A  DISSERTATION  PRESENTED  TO  THE  GRADUATE  COUNCIL 

OF  THE  UNIVERSITY  OF  FLORIDA  IN 

PARTIAL  FULFILLMENT  OF  THE  REQUIREMENTS 

FOR  THE  DEGREE  OF  DOCTOR  OF  PHILOSOPHY 


UNIVERSITY  OF  FLORIDA 
1983 


To  my  wife  Carole  and  my  daughter  Danielle 
and  to  my  parents 


ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS 

Many  individuals  have  generously  contributed  to  this  work,  and 
while  these  individuals  deserve  much  of  the  credit  for  its  completion, 
any  faults  which  may  remain  are  my  own.   I  thank  Dr.  D.  A.  Dewsbury  for 
his  support  and  suggestions,  for  providing  a  critical  scientific 
atmosphere  in  which  to  conduct  research,  and  for  his  example  as  a 
scientist.   I  would  also  like  to  thank  Drs.  H.  J.  Brockmann,  C. 
VanHartesveldt,  and  W.  B.  Webb  for  their  encouragement  and  support,  and 
for  contributing  of  their  time  and  knowledge.   I  particularly  thank 
Dr.  M.  E.  Meyer  whose  consistant  encouragement  and  support  were  a  major 
factor  in  my  completion  of  the  Graduate  Program  and  the  present  work. 
Several  of  my  fellow  students  also  contributed  to  this  work.  My 
appreciation  to  D.  Kim  Sawrey,  Bruce  Ferguson,  Larry  Shapiro,  Allen 
Hodges,  and  Denis  Baumgardner,  for  their  support,  encouragement  and 
critisms.   I  would  especially  like  to  thank  Dean  Williams  for  his  time, 
and  for  his  patience  in  explaining  some  of  the  mysteries  of  electronics 
to  a  neophyte.  Our  animal  caretakers,  T.  C.  Fryer  and  I.  Washington, 
also  deserve  my  appreciation  for  the  excellent  care  they  have  given  my 
animals.  Last,  but  most  of  all,  I  would  like  to  thank  my  wife  Carole 
who  has  supported  me  through  the  bad  times  and  the  good,  and  my  daughter 
Daniel le  whose  patience  and  understanding  exceed  her  years. 

This  research  was  funded  In  part  through  NSF  Grant  BNS78-05173  to 
Dr.  D.  A.  Dewsbury. 


in. 


TABLE  OF  CONTENTS 

PAGE 

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Ml 

ABSTRACT. v  I 

SECTION 

I  INTRODUCTION 1 

Aggression  as  a  Factor  In  Mate  Selection 4 

Fami  I  iarity  as  a  Factor  in  Mate  Selection 11 

K  i  n  Fam  i  I  I  ar  i  ty 12 

Fam i I i  ar  Others 14 

II  GENERAL  EXPERIMENTAL  CONSIDERATIONS  AND  METHODOLOGY  18 

Selection  of  Species  18 

Approaches  to  the  Study  of  Social  Preference  19 

General  Experimental  Information 22 

General  Methods  25 

Subjects  25 

Apparatus  26 

Seminatural  apparatus  26 

Preference  apparatus  29 

Aggression  apparatus  32 

I  I  I      EXPER I MENTS 35 

Seminatural  Experiments  35 

I ntroduction  35 

Subjects  36 

Proced  ure 36 

Resu Its  40 

Di  scussion 61 

Aggression  and  Fami I  iarity  Preference  Tests  63 

I n trod  uct  ion 63 

Subjects  63 

Proced  ure 64 

Results 67 

Discussion "78 


IV 


S !  b  1 1  ng  Preference  Tests 81 

Introduction 81 

Subjects 82 

Proced  ure 82 

Resu  Its 83 

Discussion 90 

GENERAL  DISCUSSION 92 

Aggressive  Ability  as  a  Factor  in  Preference 93 

Ecology,  Mating  System,  and  Aggressive  Ability 94 

Peromyscus  maniculatus 95 

Peromyscus  pol  ionotus 101 

Aggress i  on  and  Mate  Se  I  ect i on 1 06 

Intraspecif  ic  aggression 106 

Interspecific  aggression 108 

Ear  I  y  breed  i  ng 1 09 

Bruce  effect 109 

Heritable  aggressive  abi  I  ity 111 

Familiarity  as  a  Factor  in  Preference 113 

Prior  History 114 

Ecology  and  Social  System 115 

Peromyscus  maniculatus 115 

Peromyscus  pol  ionotus 117 

Kinship  as  a  Factor  in  Preference 119 

Inbreeding  in  Peromyscus  maniculatus 120 

Evidence  for  and  against  inbreeding  120 

Ecological  and  social  factors 121 

Inbreeding  in  Peromyscus  pol  Ionotus 125 

Evidence  for  and  against  inbreeding  125 

Ecological  and  social  factors 126 

Evolution  of  Monogamy  in  Peromyscus  Pol ionotus 129 

S  ummary 133 

REFERENCES 1 37 

BIOGRAPHICAL  SKETCH  162 


Abstract  of  Dissertation  Presented  to  the  Graduate  Council 
of  the  University  of  Florida  in  Partial  Fulfillment  of  the 
Requirements  for  the  Degree  of  Doctor  of  Philosophy 


AGGRESSION  AND  FAMILIARITY  AS  FACTORS  IN 

MATE  SELECTION  IN  Peromyscus  pol ionotus 

AND  Peromyscus  maniculatus 

By 

Daniel  George  Webster 

April  1983 


Chairman:  Donald  A.  Dewsbury 

Major  Department:   Psychology 

Aggressive  ability  and  familiarity  were  examined  as  factors  in 
the  social  preference  and  mate  selection  of  males  and  females  of  the 
monogamous  species  P*.  pol  ionotus.  and  the  polygamous  species  P.. 
maniculatus.  The  aggressive  behaviors  and  nesting  behavior  of  £,. 
pol ionotus  were  observed  in  a  semi  natural  apparatus;  factors  assessed 
were  1)  familiarity  based  on  cohabitation,  and  2)  aggressive  ability 
as  determined  through  aggressive  interactions.  Groups  observed  were 
composed  of  either  two  pairs  of  familiar  opposi te-sexed  individuals, 
or  two  unfamiliar  animals  of  each  sex.  Preferences  of  both  species 
were  assessed  in  an  automated  preference  apparatus.   In  addition  to 
the  two  factors  assessed  in  the  semi  natural  apparatus,  the  effects  of 
familiarity  based  on  relatedness  were  assessed  in  the  preference 
apparatus.   Measures  recorded  were  the  number  and  duration  of  visits 


vi 


to  stimulus  animals.   In  the  semi  natural  setting  P.  pol ionotus  of 
both  sexes  displayed  aggression,  and  nested  more  frequently  with  the 
more  aggressive  of  two  opposi te-sexed  individuals.  Males  of  this 
species  also  exhibited  a  behavior,  aggressive  digging,  that  may 
signal  their  aggressive  status.   Peromyscus  pol  ionotus  and  £,. 
manicul atus  of  both  sexes  exhibited  evidence  of  preference  for  more 
assertive  opposi te-sexed  individuals  (high  rather  than  low  tendency 
to  interact)  in  the  preference  apparatus.  Peromyscus  maniculatus  of 
both  sexes  also  displayed  preference  for  individuals  of  the  opposite 
sex  that  they  had  previously  been  housed  with,  but  such  familiarity 
did  not  affect  preference  in  P..  pol  ionotus.  The  lack  of  a 
significant  effect  of  familiarity  on  preference  in  P.  pol ionotus  was 
consistent  with  the  nesting  behavior  of  this  species  in  the 
seminatural  apparatus.  Differences  in  the  responses  of  JL. 
manicul  atus  and  P*.  pol  ionotus  to  f  ami  I  iar  indi  vidua  I  s  may  be  based  on 
differences  in  the  opportunities  individuals  of  these  species  have  to 
use  this  factor  in  mate  selection. 

Peromyscus  pol ionotus  females  demonstrated  significant 
preference  for  siblings  over  nonsiblings,  and  males  tended  to  display 
higher  sibling  than  nonsibling  scores.   Inbreeding  in  Pj.  pol  ionotus 
may  permit  individuals  of  this  species  to  found  populations  in 
isolated  patches  of  favorable  habitat.  Lack  of  significant 
preference  by  P.  maniculatus  for  siblings  or  nonsiblings  was 
interpreted  as  due  to  competing  preference  responses  in  this  species. 


vn 


SECTION  I 
INTRODUCTION 

The  success  of  an  organism  In  leaving  a  numerous 
posterity  is  not  measured  only  by  the  number  of 
its  surviving  offspring,  but  also  by  the  quality 
or  probable  success  of  these  offspring.   It  is 
therefore  a  matter  of  importance  which  particular 
Individual  of  those  available  is  to  be  their  other 
parent. (Fisher,  1958,  p.  143) 

Few  factors  are  as  important  to  an  individual's  reproductive 

success  as  is  the  selection  of  a  mate.  Mate  selection  involves  more 

than  simply  the  identification  of  potential  partners  as  to  species  and 

sex.   In  order  for  an  individual  to  maximize  its  future  representation 

in  the  gene  pool  it  must  also  select  the  "best"  possible  mates  on  the 

basis  of  a  variety  of  other  considerations. 

There  has  been  much  theoretical  speculation  on  the  proximate  and 

ultimate  bases  of  mate  selection  and  on  the  relative  importance  of 

mate  selection  to  different  mating  systems.   In  the  last  decade  a 

considerable  effort  has  been  made  to  collect  empirical  data  on  mate 

selection,  but  large  gaps  still  remain  in  the  existing  data  which  must 

be  filled  before  many  important  theoretical  problems  can  be  resolved. 

Particularly  lacking  are  data  on  male  choice  and  data  on  mate 

selection  In  mammalian  species,  especially  those  considered  to  be 

monogamous.  The  lack  of  data  on  male  choice  probably  stems  In  large 

part  from  a  traditional  emphasis  on  female  choice  and  male-male 

competition  (Bateman,  1948;  Daly  &  Wilson,  1978;  Trivers,  1972; 


Williams,  G.  C,  1966)  and  the  belief  that  male  choice  was  either 
nonexistent  or  negligible  in  the  face  of  this  competition.  More 
recently,  however,  it  has  been  suggested  that  mate  selection  should  be 
of  some  consequence  to  males  as  well  as  females  (e.g.,  Dewsbury, 
1982c).  The  lack  of  data  on  monogamous  mammalian  species  may  be  due  In 
part  to  two  factors  that  make  it  somewhat  difficult  to  observe  these 
species:   1)  monogamy  appears  to  be  uncommon  in  mammals  (Alexander, 
1974;  Crook,  1977;  Kleiman,  1977;  Orians,  1969)  and  2)  most  mammals, 
especially  the  smaller  species,  are  largely  nocturnal  (Vaughan,  1978). 

Study  of  the  proximate  factors  involved  in  mate  selection  is 
extremely  important  to  the  resolution  of  issues  about  the  evolution  and 
relative  importance  of  mate  selection  in  various  mammalian  taxa. 
Factors  that  have  been  proposed  to  be  of  major  importance  to  mate 
selection  are  of  two  general  types:  those  related  to  the  genotype  of  a 
potential  mate,  such  as  genetic  quality  drivers,  1972,  1976;  Zahavi, 
1975),  and  relatedness  (Maynard  Smith,  1956);  and  those  related  to 
resources,  such  as  the  ability  to  accrue  resources  drivers,  1976)  and 
parental  investment  (Eateman,  1948;  Trivers,  1972).  The  relevance  of 
any  particular  factors  as  criteria  in  mate  selection  will  vary  among 
species,  between  sexes,  and  across  mating  systems,  as  a  function  of 
differences  in  a  group  of  interrelated  variables  including  ecological 
factors  (Borgia,  1979;  Emlen  &  Oring,  1977;  Halliday,  1978). 
Differences  in  the  degree  to  which  Individuals  of  various  species 
utilize  particular  factors  as  criteria  in  mate  selection  would  be 
expected  to  reflect  differences  in  the  adaptiveness  of  those  criteria 
to  mate  selection  in  those  species.   Comparative  studies  of  mate 


selection  or  social  preference  in  different  species,  therefore,  can 
provide  an  empirical  basis  for  evaluation  of  hypotheses  on  the 
importance  of  various  factors  to  mate  selection  under  different  social 
or  mating  systems.  Such  comparisons  would  be  most  effective  when  the 
species  compared  were  closely  related  (King,  1970)  so  that  the  species 
do  not  differ  in  so  many  respects  as  to  obscure  the  relationships  of 
interest. 

Two  factors  of  the  sort  that  might  be  expected  to  be  of  broad 
importance  as  criteria  for  mate  selection  across  most  species,  but  may 
be  expected  to  vary  in  importance  among  species,  are  the  aggressive 
ability  of  a  potential  mate,  and  that  animal's  degree  of  familiarity 
with  the  Individual  expressing  choice.  An  individual  of  high 
aggressive  ability  could  be  defined  as  one  that  is  highly  competent  in 
the  performance  of  aggressive  behavior  (i.e.,  displays,  threats,  and 
fights).   Individuals  of  high  aggressive  ability  would  be  expected  to 
perform  we  I  I  in  competition  for  contested  resources  and  defense  of 
mates  and/or  offspring.  Familiarity  can  be  of  two  types,  these  are  1) 
familiarity  gained  through  exposure  to  other  unrelated  individuals,  and 
2)  familiarity  with  kin.   In  the  first  sense,  familiarity  may  provide  a 
basis  for  evaluation  of  former  mates  or  a  means  of  discriminating 
between  two  potential  mates.   In  the  second  sense,  familiarity  may 
provide  a  basis  for  avoidance  of  inbreeding,  or  as  a  yardstick  for 
comparison  of  potential  mates  (Bateson,  1978,  1980). 

This  study  was  designed  to  provide  data  on  the  importance  of 
aggression  and  familiarity  as  factors  affecting  mate  preference  in 
males  and  females  of  two  closely  related  species  of  muroid  rodents  with 


different  mating  systems:  the  monogamous  species  Peromyscus  pol ionotus 
and  the  polygamous  species  Peromyscus  maniculatus.   In  light  of  the 
relative  lack  of  data  on  mate  selection  in  monogamous  species  the  major 
focus  of  this  study  is  on  L  pol ionotus.  The  discussions  that  follow 
provide  a  brief  review  of  relevant  theoretical  factors  in  mate 
selection,  and  a  demonstration  of  the  importance  of  aggression  and 
familiarity  as  factors  in  mate  selection,  with  an  emphasis  on  mammalian 
species. 

Aggression  as  a  Factor  in  Mate  Selection 
Darwin  (1859)  recognized  that  some  males  could  gain  a  reproductive 
advantage  over  other  males  by  defeating  them  in  fights  for  females.   It 
has  been  suggested  (Bateman,  1948)  that  the  evolution  of  male-male 
competition  had  its  basis  in  differences  in  male-female  strategies  of 
investment  in  gametes.   Males  are  generally  considered  to  invest  little 
energy  in  the  production  of  gametes,  whereas  females  invest  a  great 
deal  (Bateman,  1948;  Orians,  1969;  Stacey,  1982;  Trivers,  1972). 
Because  of  their  larger  investment  females  are  valuable  to  males,  and 
the  probability  that  females  will  obtain  mates  is  high,  but  their 
reproductive  success  will  be  limited  by  the  number  of  gametes  they  can 
produce.  Males,  however,  because  they  invest  little  energy  in  the 
production  of  individual  gametes,  can  afford  to  produce  large  numbers 
of  gametes— with  which  they  could  potentially  fertilize  large  numbers 
of  females.  A  male's  reproductive  success,  therefore,  may  be  greatly 
influenced  by  the  number  of  females  he  mates  with — and  males  may  be 
expected  to  compete  to  fertilize  females  (see  however,  Dewsbury,  1982c; 


Nakatsuru  &  Kramer,  1982).  This  argument  was  extended  by  Trivers 
(1972),  who  restated  It  in  a  more  general  form  based  on  the  overall 
relative  level  of  parental  investment  of  the  two  sexes,  which  he 
hypothesized  to  determine  the  intensity  of  male-male  competition  in 
species  as  well  as  the  form  of  the  mating  system  (see  however,  Kleiman 
&  Malcolm,  1981,  p.  371;  Wickler  &  Seibt,  1981).  The  level  of 
success  achieved  by  a  male  in  competitive  mating  may  often  depend  upon 
his  ability  to  dominate  other  males.  This  notion  has  been  examined  in 
a  wealth  of  studies,  recently  reviewed  by  Dewsbury  (1982b),  on  the 
relationship  between  "dominance"  and  various  aspects  of  reproduction. 
A  male's  ability  to  conquer  other  males,  however,  also  provides 
females  witn  a  basis  by  which  to  judge  him  against  other  males — a 
basis  for  "female  choice"  (Darwin,  1874).  Borgia  (1979)  has  suggested 
that  a  female's  best  indication  of  the  relative  overall  genetic  quality 
of  a  male  is  provided  through  his  aggressive  interactions  with  other 
males.  Females  choosing  aggressive  males,  or  allowing  such  males  to 
mate  with  them,  may  in  effect  be  selecting  "good  genes"  for  their 
offspring  (Maynard  Smith,  1956;  Trivers,  1972).  Selection  for  good 
genes  has  also  been  suggested  as  a  basis  for  the  evolution  of  extra- 
vagant sexually  dimorphic  characteristics  (Fisher,  1930),  as  a  basis 
for  lek  behavior  (Borgia,  1979),  and  as  a  basis  for  female  choice  in 
Drosophi la  (Partridge,  1980).   The  major  obstacle  to  the  use  of 
heritable  factors  as  a  basis  for  choice  is  the  problem  of  "using  up" 
the  genetic  variance  for  a  trait  (Krebs  &  Davies,  1981;  Maynard  Smith, 
1978).  Several  factors,  however,  have  been  suggested  to  act  to 
maintain  genetic  variance;  these  include  1)  advantages  for 


heterozygotes  (Borgia,  1979),  2)  variation  in  the  optimal  genotype  in 
space  and  time  (Krebs  &  Davies,  1981),  3)  factors  such  as  chronic 
parasitism,  that  may  result  in  cyclic  changes  in  the  optimal  genotype 
(Hamilton  &  Zuk,  1982),  and  4)  rate  of  mutation  in  polygenic  characters 
(Lande,  1976). 

The  northern  elephant  seal  (Mirounga  angustirostr i s)  provides  an 
example  of  the  importance  of  male  aggressive  ability  in  female  choice 
in  a  natural  setting.  During  the  breeding  season  males  of  this  species 
establish  dominance  hierachies  which  are  maintained  through  threat  and 
combat.  Dominant  males  guard  groups  of  females  from  other  males,  and 
account  for  the  majority  of  first  copulations  (LeBeouf,  1974;  LeBoeuf  & 
Peterson,  1969).  Females  help  insure  that  they  will  be  inseminated  by 
an  aggressive  dominant  male  by  vocalizing  loudly  if  a  subordinate  male 
attempts  to  mount.  The  dominant  male,  alerted  by  the  female,  drives 
off  the  subordinate  male  and  copulates  with  the  female  (Cox  &  LeBoeuf, 
1977).  The  authors  note  that  females  of  many  polygynous  species  might 
be  expected  to  incite  male-male  competition  in  this  manner.  Cox  (1981) 
has  observed  that  female  elephant  seals  are  less  likely  to  vocalize  if 
the  male  mounting  them  has  just  displayed  dominance  over  another  male 
and  suggested  that  females  may  thus  select  for  males  that  frequently 
display  their  aggressive  ability.   Cox  hypothesized  that  in  general  "in 
species  where  social  status  of  males  is  correlated  with  their  genetic 
fitness,  female  choice  is  likely  to  be  based  on  social  signals  which 
are  used  in  competition  between  males"  (p.  197).   Similar  hypotheses 
have  been  proposed  by  Borgia  (1979)  and  Alexander  (1975).   Cox  (1981) 
has  provided  examples  of  several  species  in  which  females  appear  to  use 
male's  aggressive  signals  toward  each  other  as  a  basis  for  choice.   The 


list  consists  of  a  fairly  diverse  array  of  species  including 
territorial  birds  (Armstrong,  1973;  Thorpe,  1961),  tree  frogs  (Whitney 
&  Krebs,  1975a,  1975b),  sticklebacks  (Tinbergen,  1951),  and  a 
lek-forming  bird,  the  ruff  Phi lomachus  pugnax  (Hogan-Warburg,  1966). 

Male  aggressive  ability  need  not  be  heritable  or  manifest  at  the 
time  of  mating  to  be  an  important  factor  in  female  choice.   Male 
differences  in  aggressive  ability,  for  example,  will  be  related  to 
differences  in  their  ability  to  acquire  and  hold  territories  (Brown,  J. 
L.,  1964).  A  female  choosing  a  male  with  a  superior  territory  is  in 
effect  also  choosing  a  male  that  has  been  able  to  first  obtain  that 
territory  in  competition  with  other  males,  and  further  to  maintain  it 
in  the  face  of  threats  from  other  males. 

Female  preference  for  aggressive  males  has  also  been  demonstrated 
in  laboratory  choice  tests.  Sexually  experienced  female  brown  lemmings 
(Lemmus  tr imucronatus)  in  estrus  were  found,  in  olfactory  choice  tests, 
to  prefer  dominant  over  subordinate  males  (Huck,  Banks,  &  Wang,  1981). 
Female  preference  was  also  predictive  of  male  performance  in  later 
dominance  tests;  estrous  females  again  preferred  dominant  males  while 
diestrous  females  preferred  subordinate  males.  The  authors  found  that 
dominant  males  had  heavier  testes  and  higher  testosterone  levels  than 
subordinates,  and  they  hypothesized  that  female  choice  might  be  based 
on  differences  in  androgen  dependent  male  odors.  Estrous  females  were 
also  found  to  exhibit  more  copulation  with  dominant  males  in 
tether-choice  tests  (Huck  &  Banks,  1982). 

Costanzo  and  Renfrew  (1977)  studied  the  preference  of  sexually 
experienced  and  naive  female  rats  for  dominant  and  subordinate  male 


odors.  Ovariectomized  sexually  naive  females  displayed  no  preference 
in  hormonal ly  induced  estrus.  Ovariectomized  sexually  experienced 
females  preferred  dominant  males  when  not  injected  with  hormones,  but 
subordinate  males  when  in  hormonal ly  induced  estrus. 

As  discussed  earlier  a  female  may  often  invest  relatively  more  than 
a  male  in  offspring,  and  a  female's  reproductive  success  may  often  be 
more  limited  than  a  male's.  Because  of  these  factors  females  are 
generally  considered  to  be  more  choosey  than  males  when  selecting  mates 
(Bateman,  1948;  Burley,  1977,  1981;  Daly  &  Wilson,  1978;  Trivers, 
1972;  Williams,  G.  C. ,  1966)  and  the  majority  of  studies  of  social  or 
mate  preference  have  been  studies  of  female  choice.  Male  reproductive 
success,  however,  depends  on  not  only  the  number  of  mates  males  acquire 
but  the  quality  of  these  mates  as  well  (Wade,  1979).  Ralls  (1976)  has, 
for  example,  suggested  that  larger  females  may  often  be  better  mothers. 
Factors  such  as  this  might  be  considered  trivial  if  males  were  capable 
of  inseminating  an  unl  imited  number  of  females.  However,  although 
males  appear  to  produce  an  enormous  number  of  sperm  and  therefore  to 
be  capable  of  inseminating  an  enormous  number  of  females,  these  sperm 
are  emitted  as  ejaculates — of  which  a  male  may  produce  only  limited 
numbers  (Dewsbury,  1982c;  Nakatsuru  &  Kramer,  1982).   Mate  selection 
may,  therefore,  often  be  of  some  consequence  to  males  as  well  as 
females  (Dewsbury,  1982c). 

The  aggressive  ability  of  females  may  be  an  important  criterion  for 
male  choice.  Females  with  high  aggressive  ability  may  be  more  capable 
than  females  with  low  ability  in  such  behaviors  as  defense  of  young  or 
nest  sites,  or  in  competition  for  food  items.   Males  of  some  species 


have  also  demonstrated  an  ability  to  discriminate  the  aggressive  status 
of  other  individuals,  and  therefore  the  potential  to  use  this  factor  in 
mate  selection.  Male  rats,  for  example,  spend  more  time  investigating 
odor  from  a  dominant  male  than  from  a  subordinate  (Krames,  Cam,  & 
Bergman,  1969).  Male  mice  also  discriminate  between  the  odors  of 
dominant  and  subordinate  males  (Carr,  Martorano,  &  Krames,  1970)  and 
investigate  areas  marked  by  dominant  males  less  than  those  marked  by 
subordinate  males  (Jones  &  Nowell,  1973).  Both  male  and  female 
saddle-backed  tamarins  investigate  dominant  male  scent  marks  more  than 
those  of  subordinate  males  (Epple,  1973,  1974). 

Although  the  majority  of  preference  studies  to  date  have  been 
conducted  with  polygamous  species,  there  is  no  reason  to  believe  that 
members  of  monogamous  species  should  be  any  less  adept  at 
discriminations  based  on  differences  in  aggressive  status,  or  that  the 
ability  to  make  such  discriminations  should  be  any  less  useful  to 
members  of  monogamous  species.  Aggression  serves  many  of  the  same 
functions  in  monogamous  species  as  in  polygamous  species.  Although 
male-male  competition  and  aggression  near  the  time  of  copulation  may  be 
less  important  in  monogamous  than  in  many  non-monogamous  species, 
aggression  serves  many  functions  at  other  times  in  an  organism's 
lifetime,  and  success  in  these  other  aggressive  encounters  will  be  just 
as  important  to  monogamous  as  to  polygamous  males.   For  example, 
defense  of  resources  critical  to  raising  young,  and  in  many  cases  to 
initially  attracting  a  mate,  is  hypothesized  by  some  authors  to  be 
universally  displayed  by  males  of  monogamous  species.  Kleiman  (1977) 
states  that  "The  male's  territorial  defense,  which  prevents  the  over- 


10 


utilization  of  necessary  resources,  is  practiced  by  males  of  all 
monogamous  species"  (p.  54).  Kleiman  (1977)  has  also  indicated  that 
females  of  monogamous  mammalian  species  may  commonly  be  as  involved  as 
males  in  territorial  defense  and  contrasts  this  behavior  with  a  lack 
of  territorial  defense  by  non-monogamous  females.  This  hypothesis  is 
similar  to  one  previously  proposed  by  Wilson  (1975)  as  one  of  the 
biasing  ecological  conditions  for  monogamy  -  that  two  adults  are 
required  to  defend  valuable  resources  contained  in  the  territory.   If 
monogamous  females  help  to  acquire  and  defend  resources  then  monogamous 
males  may  do  well  to  choose  aggressive  females  as  mates. 

An  additional  consideration,  related  to  the  possible  function  of 
aggression  as  a  factor  in  mate  selection  in  monogamous  species,  is  the 
"early  breeding"  hypothesis  proposed  by  Darwin  (1874).  According  to 
this  theory  the  most  healthy,  more  dominant  members  of  a  species  will 
come  into  breeding  condition  earlier  in  the  season,  and  establish 
territories  earlier,  than  the  less  vigorous  subordinate  individuals. 
The  more  dominant  individuals  of  each  sex  should  then  choose  each  other 
as  the  "better"  mates  in  preference  to  subordinate  individuals.  Two 
species  whose  behavior  may  support  the  occurrence  of  this  form  of 
selection  are   the  artic  skua  (Stercorar ius  parasiticus,)  and  the 
mourning  dove  (Zenaidura  macroura) .   Individuals  generally  pair  for 
life  in  the  arctic  skua  (O'Donald,  1959).  Darker  males  of  this  species 
tend  to  breed  earlier  than  birds  of  light  or  intermediate  phenotype, 
and  females  breeding  for  the  first  time  are  more  successful  with  dark 
males.  Early  breeding  and  large  territory  size  are  both  correlated 
with  early  hatching,  and  success  of  new  pairs  with  dark  males  may  be 


11 


related  to  male  ability  to  hold  large  territories  (Davis  &  O'Donald, 
1976).  Dominant  male  mourning  doves  prefer  dominant  females  in 
experiments  with  penned  populations  (Goforth  &  Baskett,  1971). 
Dominant  pairs  breed  earlier  than  subordinate  pairs,  and  more  offspring 
of  dominant  pairs  survive. 

Based  on  theoretical  considerations,  and  on  the  results  of 
research  conducted  to  date,  predictions  can  be  made  as  to  the  relative 
importance  of  aggression  as  a  factor  In  mate  selection  in  species  with 
polygamous  and  monogamous  mating  systems.  Males  that  are  above  average 
in  their  aggressive  abilities  should  generally  be  preferred  as  mates  by 
females  of  monogamous  and  polygamous  species.  Females  of  monogamous 
species,  because  they  may  form  long-term  pair  bonds  and  consequences  of 
their  choice  may  therefore  be  more  long-term,  might  be  expected  to 
display  stronger  preference  than  polygamous  females.  Males  of 
monogamous  species  might  be  expected  to  display  preference  for  females 
with  higher  aggressive  ability  because,  among  other  considerations, 
such  females  may  be  in  breeding  condition  earlier  and  might  be  expected 
to  better  share  responsibility  for  territorial  defense.  Polygamous 
males  might  also  prefer  females  with  higher  aggressive  ability  because 
such  females  may  be  better  able  to  defend  young  or  resources  necessary 
for  raising  young,  but  preference  should  not  be  as  critical  for  these 
males  as  for  monogamous  males. 

Familiarity  as  a  Factor  In  Mate  Selection 
Familiarity  could  be  an  important  factor  In  mate  selection  In 
several  ways.   It  would  be  expected,  for  example,  that  early 
familiarity  with  conspecifics  should  aid  Individuals  in  discriminating 


12 


between  members  of  their  own  and  other  species.  Familiarity  could 
additionally  be  an  important  factor  in  recognition  of  kin,  and 
therefore  a  factor  of  importance  in  kin-selection  and  in  avoidance  of 
inbreeding.   It  may  also  be  of  importance  to  animals  of  many  species  to 
be  capable  of  discriminating  between  unrelated  strange  and  familiar 
individuals.  Two  aspects  of  familiarity,  recognition  of  kin  and  of 
familiar  others,  will  be  discussed  further. 
Kin  Fami I iarity 

Bateson  (1978,  1980)  has  hypothesized  that  early  experience  with 
kin  imprints  individuals  to  aspects  of  both  kin  and  species,  and  that 
such  kin  familiarity  is  important  to  selection  of  mates  of  the 
appropriate  species  and  to  avoidance  of  inbreeding  at  sexual  maturity. 
Avoidance  of  inbreeding  is  an  important  consideration  in  mate  selection 
because  inbreeding  often  leads  to  inbreeding  depression — a  reduction 
in  the  viability  of  inbred  offspring  and/or  of  their  ability  to 
reproduce.   Inbreeding  depression  has  been  documented  in  a  variety  of 
species  from  ungulates  (Ralls,  Brugger,  &  Bal lou,  1979)  to  rodents 
(Hill,  1974),  birds  (Bulmer,  1973),  and  Drosophi la  (Maynard  Smith, 
1956).   In  many  mammalian  species  inbreeding  is  avoided  in  part  because 
individuals  of  one  sex  emigrate  from  the  natal  group  before  sexual 
maturity.  This  type  of  emigration  has  been  observed  in  chimpanzees 
(Pusey,  1980),  lions  (Bertram  1975,  1976),  olive  baboons  (Packer, 
1979),  black-tailed  prairie  dogs  (Hoogland,  1982),  and  a  variety  of 
other  species.   Intergroup  transfer  of  individuals  in  mammalian  species 
and  the  significance  of  this  behavior  to  avoidance  of  inbreeding  have 
recently  been  discussed  by  Packer  (1979).  Emigration  from  natal  groups 


13 


may  be  mediated  by  adult  aggression  in  many  species  (e.g.,  langurs: 
Sugiyama,  1965;  elephant-shrews:  Rathbun,  1979).  However,  female 
(and  male?)  choice  has  also  been  suggested  as  a  factor  (Hoogland,  1982; 
Wittenberger,  1981)  since  individuals  should  be  selected  to  emigrate 
from  natal  groups  if  1)  relatives  refuse  to  mate  with  them  and  2) 
alternative  mates  are  not  readily  available.   In  at  least  some  rodent 
species  reproductive  maturity  may  be  inhibited  by  pheromones  produced 
by  adults  (Bediz  &  Whitsett,  1979;  Drickamer,  1979;  Lawton  & 
Whitsett,  1979;  Lombardi  &  Whitsett,  1980).  For  young  of  such  species 
emigration  from  the  natal  group  may  provide  the  major  opportunity  for 
reproduction. 

Although  It  would  appear  that  avoidance  of  inbreeding  should 
generally  be  the  rule,  certain  circumstances  may  favor  inbreeding.   If 
Inbreeding  were  not  detrimental,  individuals  could  increase  their 
inclusive  fitness  through  mating  with  relatives  (Bengtsson,  1978; 
Maynard  Smith,  1978).  Female  control  of  the  sex  of  offspring  in  the 
wasp  Euodynerus  foraminatus  may  be  used  to  counterbalance  the 
detrimental  effects  of  inbreeding  and  allow  individuals  of  this 
species  to  take  advantage  of  the  increase  in  relatedness  resulting  from 
inbreeding  (Cowan,  1979).  Bengtsson  (1978)  hypothesized  that  it  would 
be  adaptive  for  individuals  to  Inbreed  if  the  costs  of  inbreeding  were 
lower  than  the  costs  that  would  be  incurred  in  dispersal  or  in 
competition  for  mates  in  the  natal  group.  Costs  incurred  through 
dispersal  would  include  such  factors  as  increased  exposure  to  predators 
and  to  unfavorable  environmental  conditions.   Inbreeding  may  also  at 
times  be  suited  to  specialized  environmental  situations;  as  noted  by 


14 


Mayer  (1970)  "An  outbreeder  may  also  be  so  well  buffered  that  it 
stagnates  evol utionari ly.  At  the  other  end  is  the  extreme  inbreeder 
which  has  found  a  lucky  genotypic  combination  that  permits  it  to 
flourish  in  a  specialized  environmental  situation  ...  (p.  245). 
Shields  (1982)  compared  the  advantages  and  disadvantages  of 
outbreeding,  inbreeding,  and  asexual  reproduction  and  concluded  that 
inbreeding  is  often  more  advantageous  than  commonly  assumed  and  should 
be  "expected  to  be  common  in  organisms  produced  by  stable  lineage- 
environment  associations"  (p.  274). 

Although  inbreeding  may  be  adaptive  under  particular  circumstances 
individuals  of  most  species  should,  given  a  choice,  prefer  to  breed 
with  nonsiblings  rather  than  siblings.  Some  support  is  lent  to  this 
statement  by  the  observation  that  the  initiation  of  breeding  in  sibling 
pairs  is  often  delayed  in  comparison  to  the  initiation  of  breeding  in 
nonsibling  pairs  (Batzli,  Getz,  &  Hurley,  1977;  Dewsbury,  1982a; 
Hill,  1974;  McGuire  &  Getz,  1981).  The  contribution  of  preference  per 
se  to  these  findings  is,  however,  difficult  to  assess.  Animals  in 
these  studies  were  not  allowed  a  choice  of  mates  and  delayed 
reproduction,  or  lack  of  reproduction,  may  result  from  several  factors 
(Dewsbury,  1982a)  in  addition  to  preference.   It  would  be  expected  that 
reproductively  mature  males  and  females  of  most  species  should  prefer 
to  associate  with  nonsiblings  rather  than  siblings,  and  that  this 
preference  should  be  apparent  in  choice  tests. 
Fami I iar  Others 

Differences  in  familiarity  need  not  only  be  defined  in  terms  of 
differences  in  relatedness,  differences  in  familiarity  may  also  be 


15 


defined  in  terms  of  differences  in  the  amount  and  type  of  contact  an 
individual  has  had  with  others.  Familiarity  in  this  sense  is  an 
important  factor  in  many  aspects  of  an  animal's  behavior,  including 
mate  choice.   Individuals  should  improve  their  reproductive  success 
by  retaining  mates  that  they  have  previously  bred  successfully  with 
and  choosing  unfamiliar  others  over  mates  with  which  breeding  has 
previously  failed.   Coulson  (1966),  for  example,  found  kittiwakes 
(Rissa  tridactyla)  were  much  more  likely  to  change  mates  if  breeding 
had  been  unsuccessful;  and  red-billed  gulls  (Larus  novaehol landiae 
scopul inus)  exhibit  similar  behavior  (Mills,  1973).   Although  some 
authors  (e.g.,  Hal  I iday,  1978)  have  indicated  that  mate  choice  based 
on  previous  reproductive  performance  should  only  be  of  importance  to 
species  which  pair  bond  for  more  than  one  season,  all  that  is 
actually  required  is  an  ability  to  recognize  previous  mates.  This 
ability  has  been  demonstrated  in  several  species  including  rats 
(Carr,  Demesqui ta-Wander,  Sachs,  &  Maconi,  1979;  Carr,  Hirsch,  2, 
Balazs,  1980;  Krames,  Costanzo,  &  Carr,  1967),  lemmings  (Huck  & 
Banks,  1979)  and  prairie  voles  (Ward,  Baumgardner,  &  Dewsbury,  1981). 
The  ability  to  recognize  previous  mates  may  also  enhance  reproduction 
through  allowing  earlier  breeding.  This  function  of  familiarity 
has  generally  been  stressed  for  monogamous  species  (e.g.,  Daly  & 
Wilson,  1978;  Wilson,  1975). 

The  advantages  of  familiarity  to  mate  selection,  based  on 
reproductive  performance  or  early  breeding,  are  related  to  an 
individuals  previous  breeding  experience.  Familiarity  may,  however, 
also  bias  selection  of  mates  by  sexually  inexperienced  individuals. 


16 


Females  may,  for  example,  require  a  male  to  exhibit  some  evidence  of 
"commitment"  to  forming  a  pair-bond  prior  to  copulation.  The  male 
may  fill  this  requirement  by  investing  a  large  amount  of  his  time  in 
the  relationship,  and  thus  preclude  his  finding  another  female 
(Maynard  Smith,  1977),  and/or  by  demonstrating  his  ability  to  provide 
resources  (e.g.,  Nisbet,  1973).  Evidence  of  commitment  is  likely  to 
be  most  important  to  members  of  monogamous  species,  especially  those 
in  which  individuals  form  prolonged  or  lifelong  bonds,  because  many 
individuals  of  these  species  may  only  choose  a  mate  once  in  their 
lifetime.   Individuals  of  species  that  form  prolonged  pair  bonds 
might  also  be  expected  to  be  more  "prepared"  (Sel  igman,  1970)  to 
recognize  differences  in  familiarity,  than  would  be  individuals  of 
species  that  do  not  pair  bond,  if  familiarity  were  important  to  the 
maintenance  of  pair  bonds. 

Many  authors  have  indicated  that  males  of  polygamous  species 
should  mate  with  as  many  different  individuals  as  possible  (Adler, 
1978;  Bateman,  1948;  Dawk  ins,  1976;  Williams,  G.  C.,  1966;  Zucker  & 
V/ade,  1968).  Although  as  noted  previously  males  may  have  a  limited 
capacity  to  mate  and  should  therefore  be  somewhat  selective  when 
allowed  a  choice  of  partners  (Dewsbury,  1982c;  Makatsuru  &  Kramer, 
1982),  it  may  still  often  be  to  a  males  advantage  to  obtain 
additional  matings  if  the  opportunity  is  presented.  Familiarity  may 
therefore  be  of  importance  (at  least  to  polygamous  males)  in 
identification  of  females  a  male  has  already  mated  with.  However, 
because  mate  infidelity  could  have  serious  consequences  for 
monogamously  mated  individuals  (Grafen  &  Sibly,  1978;  Trivers,  1972), 


17 


it  is  likely  that  individuals  of  monogamous  species  have  been 
selected  to  detect  potential  philanderers,  and  to  select  against  such 
individuals  as  mates  (e.g.,  Erickson  &  Zenone,  1976).   It  may  be 
expected  therefore  that  polygamous  males  would  be  more  likely  than 
monogamous  males  to  prefer  novel  over  familiar  partners.  This 
prediction  is  consistent  with  previous  suggestions  that  males  of 
monogamous  species  should  be  less  likely  than  males  of  polygamous 
species  to  exhibit  a  "Coolidge  effect"  (Thomas  &  Birney,  1979; 
Wilson,  Kuehn,  &  Beach,  1963;  but  also  see  Dewsbury,  1981a,b). 
As  a  general  set  of  predictions  it  might  be  expected  that 
individuals  of  monogamous  species  would  display  greater  preference  for 
familiar  individuals  than  would  individuals  of  polygamous  species. 
Because  of  differences  in  the  consequences  of  choice  it  might  also  be 
expected  that  females  would  display  stronger  preference  than  males 
(e.g.,  Burley,  1981).   Monogamous  females  would  be  expected  to  display 
the  strongest  preference  for  familiar  individuals.  Monogamous  males 
should  be  expected  to  display  some  preference  for  familiar 
individuals,  as  might  also  polygamous  females  (unless  greater  benefits 
result  from  producing  multiply  sired  litters).  Polygamous  males, 
however,  due  to  a  greater  possibility  of  increasing  their  reproductive 
success  through  mating  with  more  than  one  female,  may  be  expected  to 
display  some  preference  for  novel  individuals  of  the  opposite  sex. 


SECTION  I  I 
GENERAL  EXPERIMENTAL  CONSIDERATIONS  AND  METHODOLOGY 

The  discussions  In  this  section  provide  a  brief  rationale  for  the 
choice  of  the  particular  species  and  experimental  procedures  that  were 
followed  In  this  study.  This  section  provides  general  methodological 
Information  common  to  all  experiments  in  this  study,  and  a  description 
of  the  apparatus  used  in  these  experiments. 

Selection  of  Species 

One  of  the  methods  that  may  be  particularly  suited  to  exposing  and 
interpreting  differences  in  social  behavior  among  species  is  the 
comparative  approach  (Dewsbury  &  Rethl Ingshafer,  1973;  King,  1970). 
Murold  rodents  are  a  group  that  Is  particularly  suited  to  the  use  of 
the  comparative  method  (Dewsbury,  1974,  1978).  The  two  murold  rodent 
species  that  were  chosen  for  comparison  in  this  study,  the  monogamous 
Peromyscus  pol ionotus  (oldfleld  mouse)  and  polygamous  Peromyscus 
manlculatus  (deer  mouse),  are  both  members  of  the  manlculatus  species 
group  of  the  subgenus  Peromyscus.  Because  the  majority  of  mammalian 
species  are  considered  to  be  non-monogamous  (Alexander,  1974;  Crook, 
1977;  Kleiman,  1977;  Orians,  1969)  and  relatively  little  Information 
is  available  about  mate  choice  In  monogamous  mammalian  species,  It  was 
considered  to  be  particularly  important  that  one  of  the  species 
selected  for  comparison  In  the  present  study  be  a  monogamous  species. 
Although  monogamy  has  been  suggested  for  several  rodent  species, 


18 


19 


"except  for  P,  pol ionotus.  the  data  are  circumstantial"  (Foltz,  1981a, 
p.  665)  and  are  open  to  more  than  one  interpretation.  Data  supportive 
of  monogamy  in  P_,.  pol  ionotus  include  the  consistent  finding  that  the 
majority  of  reproductively  mature  individuals  are  captured  as 
heterosexual  pairs  (Blair,  1951;  Foltz,  1981a;  Rand  &  Host,  1942; 
Smith,  1966),  and  behavioral  (Blair,  1951)  and  electrophoretic  (Foltz, 
1981a)  evidence  that  pairs  form  long-term  reproductive  associations. 
Information  about  mate  selection  in  this  species  was  also  considered  to 
be  of  importance,  in  addition  to  comparative  considerations,  because 
one  subspecies,  the  beach  mouse  (£,  pol  ionotus  leucocepha.l  us.) ,  is 
presently  considered  endangered. 

In  contrast  to  £*  pol ionotus.  electrophoretic  evidence  indicates 
polygamy  for  F\  maniculatus  (Birdsall  &  Nash,  1973;  Merrltt  &  Wu, 
1975)  and  females  may  even,  on  occasion,  raise  young  communally 
(Hansen,  1957).  Dewsbury  (1981c)  suggests  that  maniculatus  may  be  even 
more  promiscuous  than  indicated  by  electrophoretic  studies  because 
these  studies  do  not  consider  the  effect  of  factors  such  as 
"differential  fertilizing  capacity"  (Lanier,  Estep,  &  Dewsbury,  1979) 
that  may  affect  estimates  of  the  number  of  matings  that  have  occurred. 
Approaches  to  the  Study  of  Social  Preference 
Ideally  social  behavior  and  social  preference  should  be  studied  in 
natural  settings.   Although  this  approach  may  be  utilized  successfully 
with  diurnal  and  highly  visible  species,  it  is  often  an  impractical,  or 
nearly  impossible,  approach  for  many  species.   As  an  alternative 
investigators  have  often  turned  to  the  study  of  populations  in  outdoor 
(Agren,  1976;  Boice,  1977;  Boice  &  Adams,  1980;  Gipps  &  Jewell, 


20 


1979;  Jannett,  1980;  Lidicker,  1980)  or  indoor  (Bowen  &  Brooks,  1978; 
Crowcroft  &  Rowe,  1963;  Getz  &  Carter,  1980;  Hill,  1977;  Poole  & 
Morgan,  1976;  Reimar  &  Petras,  1967;  Thiessen  &  Maxwell,  1979; 
Thomas  &  Birney,  1979)  seminatural  enclosures.  While  these  enclosures 
do  not  replicate  natural  conditions  in  many  respects,  they  do  allow  the 
investigator  to  approximate  some  aspects  of  the  natural  setting,  and 
allow  a  degree  of  control  over  experimental  variables  that  is  generally 
not  available  in  nature. 

Even  in  a  seminatural  apparatus,  however,  interactions  may  often 
be  so  complex  that  it  is  difficult  to  evaluate  the  effects  of  any 
single  variable  on  a  particular  behavior  such  as  social  preference. 
This  problem  has  led  investigators  to  the  use  of  even  more  controlled 
situations,  such  as  preference  apparatus  of  various  types,  to  evaluate 
the  role  of  various  factors  in  social  preference.   In  the  typical 
preference  paradigm  an  animal  (the  "choice"  animal)  is  allowed  to 
express  preference  by  "choosing"  between  two  or  more  alternative 
stimuli.  Behavioral  measures  of  preference  may  include  factors  such  as 
the  number  of  approaches,  number  of  visits,  duration  of  visits,  time 
spent  huddling  together,  mating  activity,  and  a  variety  of  other 
measures.  Use  of  preference  apparatus,  in  addition  to  allowing  more 
controlled  investigation  of  particular  factors  (e.g.,  familiarity)  than 
may  be  available  in  seminatural  apparatus,  allows  the  experimenter  to 
control  the  degree  of  contact  between  choice  animals  and  stimulus 
animals.   In  a  tether  preference  apparatus,  for  example,  stimulus 
animals  are  tethered  in  a  fixed  area  while  the  choice  animal  is  allowed 
free  access  to  the  apparatus  and  may  express  preference  through 


21 


proximity  or  contact  behaviors,  or  under  appropriate  conditions,  mating 
behavior  (Ward  et  a  I.,  1981;  Huck  &  Banks,  1982;  Webster,  Williams,  & 
Dewsbury,  1982).  An  alternative  method  used  in  preference  tests  is  to 
place  cellars  on  the  stimulus  animals,  and  then  place  these  animals  in 
compartments  with  doorways  of  a  size  large  enough  to  allow  access  by 
choice  animals,  but  too  small  for  the  collared  animals  to  pass  through 
(Mainardi,  Marsan,  &  Pasquali,  1965;  McDonald  &  Forslund,  1978). 
Direct  contact  between  choice  animals  and  stimulus  animals  may  also  be 
prevented  by  simply  constructing  stimulus  compartments  or  containers  so 
that  they  are  not  accessible  by  the  choice  animal  (Agren  &  Meyerson, 
1977;  Carmichael,  1980;  Carr,  Wylie,  &  Loeb,  1970;  Murphy,  1977; 
Webster,  Sawrey,  Williams,  &  Dewsbury,  1982).  Experimenters  have  also 
opted  at  times  to  test  preference  for  odors  from  stimulus  animals 
rather  than  using  the  animals  themselves  (Carr  et  al.,  1980;  Fass, 
Guterman,  &  Stevens,  1978;  Gilder  &  Slater,  1978;  Huck  &  Banks,  1979, 
1980;  Krames  et  al.,  1967;  Ruddy,  1980),  or  to  restrict  choice  cues 
to  olfactory  cues  by  us<ng  anesthetized  stimulus  animals  (Landauer, 
Banks,  &  Carter,  1977;  Landauer,  Seidenberg,  &  Santos,  1978;  Murphy, 
1980). 

While  preference  apparatus  offer  the  opportunity  for  greater 
control  over  variables  than  do  semi  natural  apparatus,  the  conditions 
under  which  preference  is  assessed  do  not  approximate  natural 
conditions  as  closely  as  do  conditions  in  seminatural  apparatus.  With 
preference  apparatus,  therefore,  one  may  run  a  greater  risk  of 
obtaining  results  that  are  misleading  in  respect  to  behavior  under  more 
natural  conditions.  Social  preferences  may,  for  example,  sometimes  be 


22 


expressed  less  strongly  in  preference  apparatus  than  they  would  be 
In  a  more  natural  context;  one  might  therefore  be  more  likely  to 
falsely  reject  a  factor  as  unimportant  to  social  preference  in  these 
tests.  One  way  to  minimize  this  problem  is  to  first  assess  a  species' 
social  behavior  in  more  natural  settings,  such  as  in  seminatural 
apparatus,  and  select  factors  for  preference  experiments  on  the  basis 
of  those  results.  Alternatively  one  might  use  results  from  seminatural 
experiments  in  pert  as  a  guide  in  interpretation  of  results  from 
preference  experiments. 

General  Experimental  Information 

This  study  was  designed  to  provide  data  on  aggression  and 
familiarity  as  factors  in  the  social  preference  of  monogamous  and 
polygamous  species.  Partial  data  are  available  about  the  function  of 
these  factors  in  the  social  preference  of  the  representative  species 
chosen  for  this  study,  Pj.  pol  ionotus  and  £».  manicul  atus. 

Available  evidence  indicates  that  aggression  may  be  an  important 
factor  in  the  social  behavior  of  P,.  manicul atusf  that  more  aggressive 
males  may  sire  more  offspring  than  less  aggressive  males,  and  that 
differences  in  male  aggressive  ability  may  be  important  in  female 
choice  in  this  species.   In  P_».  maniculatus  blandus  Blair  and  Howard 
(1944)  found  that,  in  experimental  populations  consisting  of  two 
individuals  of  each  sex,  one  male  would  generally  establish  dominance 
over  the  other.  The  dominant  male  generally  nested  with  both  females 
more  frequently  than  did  the  subordinate,  and  the  authors  were  able  to 
establish  (throuah  coat-color  markers)  that  dominant  males  sired  the 


23 


majority  (19  of  21)  of  litters  In  their  study.  Dewsbury  (1979,  1981c) 
found  that  male  dominance  In  E*  maniculatus  balrd.i  was  positively 
related  to  copulatory  behavior,  and  that  dominant  males  not  only 
copulated  more  than  subordinates,  but  that  they  also  sired  a  larger 
number  of  offspring  (Dewsbury,  1981c).  Eisenberg  (1962)  observed  that 
after  the  formation  of  dominance  relationships  between  male  E*. 
maniculatus  gambel i  i ,  females  of  this  species  that  had  been  paired  with 
subordinate  males  for  two  weeks  prior  to  aggression  tests  generally 
failed  to  remain  with  their  subordinate  male  partners  and  nested 
Instead  with  the  dominant  male. 

Blair  and  Howard  (1944)  studied  two  subspecies  of  Em.   pol Ionotus, 
P.  pol Ionotus  albifrons  and  P.  pol Ionotus  Iftucocephal us,  and  found 
little  evidence  of  aggression  against  conspeclfics  by  Individuals  of 
either  sex.   In  addition  all  four  individuals  (two  males  and  two 
females)  In  a  group  were  frequently  found  nesting  together.  From  these 
observations  the  authors  concluded  that  Pj.  pol Ionotus  were  a  very 
social  species.  Field  observations,  however,  do  not  support  the  notion 
that  adult  E^  pol ionotus  are  highly  social.  Although  £*.  pol Ionotus 
are  commonly  found  in  family  groups  composed  of  a  male,  a  female,  and 
young  (Blair,  1951;  Foltz,  1979;  Rand  &  Host,  1942;  Smith,  1966; 
personal  observations),  sexually  mature  Individuals  of  the  same  sex  are 
never  (Smith,  1966),  or  very  infrequently  (Blair,  1951;  Rand  &  Host, 
1942)  found  together  in  the  same  nest.   In  addition  Blair  (1951) 
observed  wounding  in  some  transient  and  immature  Individuals  and  also 
observed,  In  trap  and  release  experiments,  that  adult  females  often 
chased  other  females  from  nests.  Smith  (1967)  has  suggested  that 


24 


females  of  this  species  "are  normally  dominant  over  their  mates  and 
play  a  major  role  in  the  process  of  pair  formation  and  maintenance  of 
the  pair  bond"  (p.  236).  JL.  pol ionotus  have  also  been  observed  to 
exhibit  aggression  in  some  laboratory  tests  (Garten,  1976;  Smith, 
Garten,  &  Ramesy,  1975),  but  the  conditions  for  these  tests  do  not 
allow  evaluation  of  the  function  of  aggression  in  a  social  context,  or 
as  a  factor  in  social  preference. 

Few  data  are  available  on  the  function  of  aggression  and 
familiarity  in  the  social  behavior  of  P,  pol ionotus.  or  in  the 
social  behavior  of  monogamous  species  in  general.  The  first  set  of 
experiments  in  this  study  was  designed  to  provide  such  data.  These 
experiments  were  conducted  in  a  seminatural  apparatus  that  was  designed 
with  artificial  burrows.  This  design  takes  into  consideration  the 
semifossorial  habits  of  FV,.  pol  ionotus.  and  thereby  allows  an 
approximation  of  natural  conditions  in  this  species. 

The  seminatural  experiments  with  P*.  pol ionotus  were  foi  lowed  by 
preference  experiments  on  both  £*,  pol  ionotus  and  E*.   maniculatus. 
These  experiments  allowed  preference  based  on  aggressive  ability  and 
familiarity  to  be  assessed  under  the  same  conditions  for  both  species, 
and  thus  allowed  a  direct  comparison  of  the  relative  value  of  these 
factors  in  the  social  preference  of  these  two  species.  The  first  set 
of  these  experiments  examines  aggressive  ability  and  familiarity  with 
unrelated  individuals  (based  on  previous  contact)  as  factors  in  social 
preference;  the  second  experiment  examines  preference  for  siblings. 


25 


General  Methods 
Subjects 

Subjects  for  this  study  were  45  to  65  day  old  Individuals  of  two 
species  of  muroid  rodent,  Peromyscus  pol ionotus  subqriseus  and 
Peromyscus  maniculatus  bairdf .  The  P_,_  pol  ionotus  were  laboratory-bred 
animals  one  to  four  generations  removed  from  the  wild.  The  parental 
stock  was  obtained  from  two  different  subpopu lations  in  the  Ocala 
National  Forest  in  Florida.  The  first  group  of  these  animals  was 
trapped  in  1978  from  road  shoulders  along  State  Road  316  between  Salt 
Springs  and  Eureka.  Additional  animals  for  breeding  stock  were  trapped 
in  1980  from  road  shoulders  along  U.S.  Highway  19.  These  two 
populations  are  from  the  same  general  area  as  that  listed  as  population 
25  by  Selander,  Smith,  Suh,  Johnson,  and  Gentry  (1971).  The  method  of 
capture  was  similar  to  that  detailed  by  Foltz  (1979). 

It  is  not  possible  to  determine  how  many  generations  removed  from 
the  wild  the  Pj.  manicu latus  were.  This  colony  was  founded  at  the 
University  of  Florida  with  animals  obtained  from  near  East  Lansing, 
Michigan,  in  1970,  and  additional  wild  stock  has  been  added  on  several 
occasions  since. 

Animals  were  housed  In  clear  plastic  cages  measuring  48  x  27  x  13 
cm  or  29  x  19  x  13  cm  with  wood  shavings  as  bedding.  Purina  laboratory 
animal  chow  and  water  were  provided  ad  lib.  Prior  to  serving  as 
subjects  all  animals  were  maintained  as  litters.  Peromyscus  pol ionotus 
I Itters  were  weaned  at  22  or  23  days  of  age,  E*.  manicu latus  were 
weaned  at  21  days  of  age.  Animals  that  exhibited  obvious  physical 


26 


defects,  such  as  extensive  tail  wounds  or   missing  tails,  were  not 
selected  for  study. 

Animals  of  both  species  were  maintained  on  a  reversed  16L:8D 
photoperiod.  All  adaptation  and  testing  were  conducted  during  the  dark 
portion  of  the  photoperiod.  With  the  exception  of  observations 
conducted  in  the  seminatural  apparatus,  which  was  in  a  separate  room, 
all  studies  and  adaptation  periods  were  conducted  in  the  P*.  pol  iono.tus 
colony  room.  Procedural  details  specific  to  particular  studies  are 
described  in  the  methods  sections  of  those  studies. 
Apparatus 
Seminatural  apparatus 

The  seminatural  apparatus  was  a  large  square  Plexiglas  arena  125 
cm  on  a  side  and  46  cm  deep.  The  sides  of  this  arena  were  constructed 
with  1/4  inch  Plexiglas  and  the  floor  was  constructed  with  1/2  Inch 
plywood  and  painted  grey.  The  arena  was  partitioned,  with  four  85  cm 
lengths  of  1/4  inch  Plexiglas,  into  a  square  central  area  that  measured 
85  cm  on  a  side  and  four  right  angle  triangular  corner  compartments 
with  sides  of  85  cm,  61  cm,  and  61  cm  (See  Figure  1). 

Two  nest  boxes  were  attached  to  each  corner  compartment.  Nest 
boxes  were  constructed  with  sides  of  )/4    inch  Plexiglas  and  1/4  inch 
plywood  backs.  They  measured  1 0  x  9  x  8  cm  and  had  hinged  Plexiglas 
lids  to  provide  access  for  removal  of  animals  and  cleaning.  A  3.2  cm 
diameter  hole  cut  in  the  front  of  each  nest  box  provided  access  for  the 
animals.   In  each  corner  compartment  two  matching  3.2  cm  diameter 
holes,  cut  in  the  sides  of  the  apparatus,  45.5  cm  from  the  corner  and 
1.5  cm  from  the  floor,  provided  access  to  the  nest  boxes.  The  front  of 


27 


28 


one  of  the  nest  boxes  for  each  corner  compartment  was  connected  with 
silicon  cement  directly  to  the  side  of  the  apparatus  in  line  with  one 
of  these  holes.  The  other  nest  box  in  each  corner  compartment  was 
connected  to  the  second  opening  by  means  of  a  48  cm  length  of  Tygon 
polyethylene  tubing  with  an  internal  diameter  of  2.5  cm  and  an  external 
diameter  of  3.2  cm,  and  thus  formed  an  artificial  burrow.  Silicon 
cement  was  used  to  attach  one  side  of  the  nest  box  to  the  apparatus  and 
to  connect  the  tubing  to  the  openings  for  the  nest  box  and  the 
apparatus. 

Animals  could  gain  access  from  the  corner  compartments  to  the 
central  area  of  the  apparatus  through  3.2  cm  diameter  holes  centered  on 
and  1.5  cm  from  the  bottom  of  the  partition  which  formed  the 
compartment.  A  2.5  cm  hole  3  cm  from  the  right  angle  corner  and  2  cm 
from  the  floor  of  the  apparatus  provided  access  for  the  drinking  tube 
of  a  water  bottle. 

The  seminatural  apparatus  was  in  a  room  separate  from  the  colony 
room  but  maintained  on  a  16L:8D  photopericd  Identical  to  that 
maintained  in  the  colony  room.  The  apparatus  was  illuminated  In  the 
light  phase  of  the  photopericd  by  four  75-watt  incandescent-white  bulbs 
and  two  60-watt  red  bulbs,  each  suspended  three  feet  above  the  floor  of 
the  apparatus,  and  during  the  dark  phase  of  the  photopericd  by  the  two 
60-watt  red  bulbs  alone. 

Behavioral  measures  were  recorded  by  means  of  a  20-channel 
Esterl Ine-Angus  event  recorder.  The  behaviors  exhibited  by  each  group 
of  animals,  during  their  four  days  In  the  seminatural  apparatus,  were 
also  recorded  on  videotape  using  a  Hitachi  CCTV  low  light  television 


29 


camera,  and  a  Panasonic  time  lapse  VTR  video  tape  recorder  set  on  a  72 
hour  record  mode. 
Preference  apparatus 

The  preference  apparatus  was  a  three  chambered  rectangular  box 
with  a  hinged  lid;  it  was  constructed  of  1/4  Inch  Plexiglas  and 
measured  44  x  21  .5  x  20  cm  (Figure  2).  The  Inside  measurements  of  the 
two  end  chambers  were  10  x  21.5  x  20  cm.  These  chambers  were  open  to 
the  central  area  through  a  7  x  7  cm  opening.  The  end  chambers  were 
designed  to  accommodate  small  removable  "choice  chambers"  which 
measured  10  x  8  x  6  cm.  A  "stimulus  box"  with  an  inside  measurement  of 
8  x  7  x  8  cm  was  attached  to  each  end  chamber,  and  was  open  to  It 
through  a  6  x  5  cm  opening.  When  choice  chambers  were  placed  In  the 
end  chambers,  therefore,  one  end  of  the  choice  chamber  was  accessible 
from  the  central  area,  while  the  other  end  was  open  to  the  stimulus 
box.  A  hardware  cloth  screen  installed  In  the  opening  to  the  stimulus 
box  and  a  second  three-sided  piece  of  hardware  cloth  which  fit  the 
Inside  of  the  stimulus  box  provided  a  "double  screen"  between  the 
choice  chamber  and  the  stimulus  box. 

A  bank  of  three  red-sensitive  photocells  (peak  response  at  735 
nm),  wired  in  a  series  behind  each  choice  chamber,  registered  entries 
to  the  chamber.  The  light  sources  for  the  photocells  were  60-watt  red 
light  bulbs  placed  27  cm  in  front  of  the  apparatus  and  directly  in 
front  of  the  bank  of  photocells.  Each  photocell  was  attached  to  the 
end  of  a  tubular  4.3  cm  piece  cut  from  a  12  x  75  cm  disposable  plastic 
culture  tube.  The  outs  Ides  of  these  tubes  were  painted  black;  this  in 
effect  columnated  the  light  to  the  photocells.  The  photocells  were 


30 


31 


a: 

LU   LU 

-J  <r 
3  o 

Q.  U. 


OJ 

o 

o 

ro 

o 

O 

o 

O 

o 

o 

o 
i 

c 

—1 

5 

LU 
Q 

Z  O 
Ld  O 
>    LU 

lu  cr 


o  or 

>-  LU 

°  s 

LU  = 


T 
Q     0  0 


o  o 
z  o 


o  o 


"O 


O- 


o 

IS 
-zr 

o  o 

2   O 


jC*_ 


Q 


Ty 


Y 

LU 

5 

n 

C  ) 

■S 

n 

<] 

C) 

-) 

cr 

0. 

to 

5    CL 

O    3 
CL    CO 


32 


situated  such  that  In  order  to  register  a  visit  to  the  choice  chamber, 
an  animal  had  to  be  completely  Inside  the  chamber,  and  at  least 
partially  In  the  half  of  the  chamber  closest  to  the  stimulus  animal. 
Each  bank  of  photocells  was  wired  In  a  series  with  the  coil  circuit  of 
a  10,000  ohm,  24  VDC  DPDT  relay  (see  Figure  3).  These  relays  were 
powered  through  output  from  a  variable  power  supply  set  for  a 
continuous  output  of  26  volts.  One  of  the  normally  closed  circuits  of 
each  of  these  relays  was  wired  into  the  pen  circuit  of  an  Ester  line- 
Angus  event  recorder;  this  provided  a  permanent  record  of  entries  into 
each  choice  chamber.  Output  from  another  normally  closed  circuit  of 
these  relays  was  used  to  control  a  second  set  of  relays  on  a  relay 
rack.  Two  banks  of  Sodeco  counters  received  input  through  the  normally 
open  contacts  of  these  relays.  The  first  bank  of  counters  was  wired  In 
a  series  to  pulse  formers  and  recorded  the  number  of  visits  to  each 
chamber  regardless  of  visit  duration.   Input  to  the  second  bank  of 
counters  was  regulated  by  means  of  a  recycling  timer  set  to  produce 
pulses  at  1/3  of  a  second.  These  counters  recorded  the  total  duration 
of  visits  to  the  nearest  1/3  of  a  second.  Session  duration  was 
automatically  controlled  via  another  timer  (not  displayed)  which 
controlled  the  input  to  the  recycling  timer  and  counters. 
Aggression  apparatus 

The  "aggression  arena"  was  constructed  from  a  large  48.5  x  38 
x  20   cm  plastic  cage.  Two  3.2  cm  diameter  holes  were  cut  in  the  two 
longer  sides  of  the  cage  centered  25.5  cm  apart  and  3.5  cm  from  the 
bottom.  Silicon  cement  was  used  to  form  a  gasket  around  each  hole. 
Matching  holes  and  gaskets  were  placed  on  one  side  of  two  48  x  27  x  13 


33 


cm  plastic  cages.  The  larger  cage  and  two  smaller  cages  could  then  be 
connected  by  6.5  cm  lengths  of  Tygon  polyethylene  tubing  (Internal 
diameter  of  2.5  cm  and  external  diameter  of  3.2  cm)  to  form  the 
"aggression  apparatus"  (See  Figure  4).  During  adaptation  procedures 
the  aggression  arena  and  smal ler  cages  were  connected  with  unobstructed 
lengths  of  tubing;  a  piece  of  metal  screen  in  the  center  of  each 
length  of  tubing  prevented  animals  from  traveling  through  the  tubes 
during  tests.  A  1/4  Inch  53.5  x  43  cm  Plexiglas  lid  was  placed  over 
the  large  cage,  and  wire  cage  lids  over  the  smaller  cages,  while 
testing  was  conducted. 


34 


25.5  cm 


U 


T7 '  r 


u 


1  i \ 


E 
o 

00 

ro 


T 

E 
o 

OJ 


48.5  cm- 


H 


n 


48  cm 


FIGURE  4  Aggression  Apparatus 


SECTION  II  I 
EXPER I MENTS 

Semi  natural  Experiments 

This  section  is  divided  into  three  major  subsections;  the  three 
sets  of  experiments  that  comprise  this  study  are  each  described  within 
separate  subsections  under  the  headings  of  Seminatural  Experiments, 
Aggression  and  Familiarity  Preference  Tests,  and  Sibling  Preference 
Tests.  Each  of  these  subsections  begins  with  a  brief  introduction  to 
the  experiments  in  that  subsection,  and  specific  information  on 
subjects  and  procedures  for  these  experiments.  This  information  is 
followed  by  the  results  of  these  experiments  and  a  brief  discussion  of 
the  results.  The  results  of  all  three  sets  of  experiments  in  this 
study  are  discussed  together,  in  the  context  of  the  ecology  of  £!_,. 
pol  ionotus  and  JL.  maniculatus  and  theoretical  considerations,  in  the 
General  Discussion  section. 
Introduction 

This  experiment  was  designed  to  provide  information  on  aggression 
and  familiarity  as  factors  in  the  social  behavior  end  mate  selection  of 
Pj.  pol  ionotus.   (Similiar  types  of  data  already  exist  for  P_j. 
maniculatus:  Blair  &  Howard,  1944;  Dewsbury,  1979,  1981c:  Eisenberg, 
1962.)   Although  it  is  difficult  to  establish  the  relevance  of  factors 
in  social  preference  per  se  with  seminatural  observations,  such  data 
can  provide  an  indication  of  the  functions  a  factor  may  serve  in 


35 


36 


nature,  and  can  provide  indications  of  whether  a  factor  may  be  of 

importance  in  social  preference. 

Subjects 

Subjects  were  40  male  and  40  female  JL.  pol Ionotus.  Pricr  to 
serving  as  subjects,  animals  were  maintained  as  previously  described  In 
the  section  on  general  methods.  Subjects  were  selected  using  the 
criteria  described  In  the  general  methods,  and  the  additional  criterion 
that  the  animals  within  any  group  had  no  common  grandparents. 
Procedure 

In  order  to  better  separate  and  evaluate  the  roles  of  aggression 
and  familiarity  in  the  social  behavior  of  P.  pol Ionotus  animals  were 
observed  under  two  different  experimental  conditions,  the  "paired" 
condition  and  the  "single"  condition.  Twenty  animals  of  each  sex  were 
assigned  to  either  the  single  condition  or  the  paired  condition. 
Animals  in  each  condition  were  divided  into  10  groups;  two  animals  of 
each  sex  were  assigned  to  each  group.  Each  of  the  10  groups  of  animals 
in  each  condition,  single  or  paired,  were  treated  separately.  Animals 
within  each  group  were  lightly  anesthetized  with  ether  and  shaved  In 
one  of  the  following  four  patterns:  (1)  band  shaved  around  the  neck; 
(2)  band  shaved  around  the  middle;  (3)  band  shaved  at  the  rear;  (4) 
no  shaved  area.  Shaving  was  performed  one  day  prior  to  beginning  the 
first  experimental  manipulation.  Approximately  equal  numbers  of  males 
and  females  received  each  shave  pattern.  Subjects  under  both  the 
single  and  paired  conditions  were  exposed  to  a  series  of  three 
different  experimental  manipulations.  These  manipulations,  in  order, 


37 


and  their  durations  were  "nest  building,"  4  days;  "seminatural 
isolation,"  3  days;  and  "seminatural  interaction,"  4  days. 

During  the  four-day  nest  building  period  animals  were  housed  in  48 
x  27  x  13  cm  plastic  cages  on  San-i-cel  bedding.  Animals  in  the  single 
groups  were  housed  individually;  animals  in  the  paired  groups  were 
housed  as  two  separate  pairs  of  opposi te-sexed  animals.  Three  2-inch 
square  "Nestlets"  (Ancare  Corp.)  were  provided  as  nesting  material  in 
each  cage.  The  type  of  nest  built  was  assessed  just  prior  to  the 
beginning  of  the  dark  period  on  the  next  4  consecutive  days.  Nests 
were  rated  as  one  of  three  types:  (0)  no  nest;  (1)  platform  nest; 
(2)  covered  nest. 

The  seminatural  isolation  period  began  in  the  first  dark  phase 
which  followed  the  nest  building  period.  Animals  were  transferred  from 
the  colony  room  to  the  room  containing  the  seminatural  apparatus 
approximately  15  min  after  the  beginning  of  the  dark  phase.  Animals 
from  single  groups  were  each  placed  individually  in  corner 
compartments,  with  animals  of  the  same  sex  in  compartments  diagonally 
opposite  each  other.  Animals  from  paired  groups,  which  had  been 
maintained  as  pairs  during  nest  building,  were  transferred  to  the 
seminatural  apparatus  in  the  same  paired  relationship.  Pairs  were 
placed  in  corner  compartments  of  the  apparatus  diagonally  opposite  each 
other.  The  opening  from  each  corner  compartment  to  the  central  area 
was  closed  with  a  solid  black  rubber  stopper  so  that  animals  were 
restricted  to  the  compartment  in  which  they  had  been  placed.   The  floor 
of  the  central  area  and  of  the  corner  compartments  had  been  covered 
with  San-i-cel  to  a  depth  of  approximately  1.5  cm;  each  corner 


38 


compartment  also  contained  three  Nest  lets,  and  food  and  water  was 

avai I  able  ad  lib. 

Animals  were  maintained  in  the  corner  compartments  for  3  days. 

Activity  during  this  entire  pericd  was  videotaped.  Each  pair  of 

animals  in  the  paired  groups  was  also  observed  for  three  alternate 

10-minute  periods  during  the  dark  phase  on  the  day  the  animals  were 

Introduced,  and  on  the  following  two  days.  On  the  first  day, 

observation  was  begun  as  soon  as  all  animals  had  been  placed  In  the 

apparatus.  On  each  of  the  fol lowing  2  days  one  of  the  pairs  was 

designated  as  the  first  pair  to  be  observed,  and  the  first  period  of 

observation  was  begun  when  the  members  of  that  pair  had  emerged  from 

their  burrow.  The  behavioral  and  aggressive  measures  that  were 

recorded  were  similar  to  categories  described  by  All  in  and  Banks  (1968) 

and  Colvin  (1973).  Measures  were  recorded  by  means  of  a  20-channel 

Esterl ine-Angus  event  recorder.  The  measures,  and  definitions  of  each, 

were  as  fol lows: 

Approach     Scored  when  an  animal  came  within  one  and 
one-half  body  lengths  of  another  while 
oriented  toward  it. 


Attack       Scored  when  one  animal  lunged  at  or  charged 
another  but  did  not  pursue  the  other  or 
initiate  vigorous  biting  behavior.  This 
behavior  could  be  accompanied  by  a  single 
bite  or  attempts  to  bite. 


Chase        Scored  when  one  animal  pursued  another. 


39 


Fight        Scored  when  one  animal's  attack  on  another 
escalated  to  vigorous  biting  behavior  by 
both  individuals.  Generally  the  initiator 
would  knock  the  other  animal  over,  or  roll 
to  one  side  with  the  other  animal  clenched 
in  Its  jaws  while  shaking  Its  head,  often 
simultaneously  clawing  with  the  rear  claws. 
Fighting  often  resulted  after  one  animal  did 
not  retreat  when  attacked,  but  rather  attempted 
to  defend  Itself  or  at  the  end  of  a  chase  If 
the  pursuing  animal  caught  the  other. 


Rough-and     A  very  vigorous  form  of  fighting;  rough-and- 
Tumble-Fight  tumble  fights  were  only  scored  when  both 
animals  were  tumbling  end  over  end  while 
attempting  to  bite  and  claw  each  other. 


Displacement  Scored  when  one  animal  retreated  upon 
another  animals  approach. 


Submissive    Scored  when  one  animal,  upon  approach  or 

attack  by  another,  either  rol led  over  on  its 
back,  or  reared  back  upon  its  hind  legs  with 
its  nose  pointed  up,  and  made  no  attempt  to 
defend  itself.  Both  approach  and  submission 
or  attack  and  submission  were  scored  for  each 
encounter. 


Aggressive    This  was  a  very  vigorous  form  of  digging 
Digging      behavior  much  more  Intense  than  the  type  of 
digging  these  animals  have  been  observed  to 
perform  in  an  isolated  test  (Webster,  Williams, 
Owens,  Geiger,  and  Dewsbury;  1981).  Although 
this  behavior  was  not  generally  directed  toward 
an  opponent,  it  was  very  similar  to  that  described 
by  Al I  In  and  Banks  (1968). 


Under  both  experimental  conditions,  single  and  paired,  the 
isolation  period  was  followed  by  the  seminatural  interaction  period. 
Ten  mln  prior  to  the  first  dark  phase  In  this  period  the  rubber 
stoppers  were  removed  from  the  partitions  between  each  corner 
compartment  and  the  central  area.  Behavioral  and  aggressive 


40 


interactions  between  animals,  as  defined  above,  were  recorded  during 
the  first  hour  of  the  dark  phase  for  4  consecutive  days.  Nesting 
relationships  were  recorded  each  day,  for  the  last  3  of  these  4  days, 
20  min  prior  to  the  beginning  of  the  dark  phase.  An  animal  was  defined 
as  having  nested  with  another  if  it  was  found  in  the  same  nest  with  the 
other,  and  videotape  records  verified  that  it  had  not  switched  nests 
between  the  period  extending  from  after  the  first  1/2  hr  of  the 
preceding  light  phase  to  1/2  hr  prior  to  the  nest  check.  Activity 
during  the  entire  isolation  and  interaction  stages  was  videotaped. 

The  seminatural  apparatus,  including  the  artificial  burrows 
and  nest  boxes,  was  thoroughly  cleaned  with  a  solution  of  Sterigent  (a 
deodorant  and  disinfectant  soap)  before  each  group  of  animals  was 
introduced  to  the  apparatus.  Water  bottles  were  also  cleaned  and 
refilled,  and  fresh  San-i-cel,  food,  and  Nestlets  were  placed  in  the 
apparatus. 
Results 

Peromyscus  pol ionotus  appeared  to  adapt  very  quickly  to  the 
seminatural  apparatus  in  general,  and  to  the  artificial  burrows  in 
particular.  All  but  five  of  the  40  individuals  in  the  paired  condition 
entered  and  explored  the  artificial  burrows  within  the  first  hour  of 
observation,  and  all  except  two  pairs  of  the  20  pairs  observed  in  the 
paired  condition  had  constructed  at  I  eat  a  rudimentary  nest  in  the 
burrow  by  the  end  of  their  first  day  in  the  apparatus.   Individuals  in 
the  paired  or  single  groups  were  only  infrequently  observed  nesting  in 
the  alternate  nest  box,  although  this  box  was  frequently  used  for 
feeding  and  as  an  escape  when  individuals  were  attacked.  Aggressive 


41 


relationships  between  Individuals  in  each  group  appeared  to  remain 
fairly  stable  over  their  four  days  together  in  the  seminatural 
apparatus.   In  all  10  single  groups,  and  in  seven  of  the  10  paired 
groups,  the  individual  with  the  highest  total  frequency  of  aggressive 
behavior  (sum  of  all  attacks,  chases,  fights,  and  rough-and-tumble 
fights)  on  day  one,  still  exhibited  the  highest  frequency  of  aggressive 
behavior  on  day  four.   In  the  other  three  paired  groups  the  Individual 
that  exhibited  the  highest  frequency  of  aggressive  behavior  on  day  two 
also  exhibited  the  highest  frequency  on  day  four. 

Each  animal  in  a  group  could  potentially  Interact  with  twice  as 
many  opposite-sexed  individuals  as  same-sexed  Individuals.  To  adjust 
for  this  bias,  the  mean  value  of  any  measure  of  an  animal's  interaction 
with  both  opposite-sexed  individuals,  rather  than  the  total,  was  used 
in  analyses  Involving  opposite-sexed  Individuals. 

Several  significant  differences  in  aggression  were  apparent  In 
comparisons  between  males  and  females  in  both  the  paired  and  single 
conditions.  Males  were  more  aggressive  than  females  in  a  statistically 
significant  larger  number  of  groups  by  all  measures  except  the  number 
of  rough-and-tumble  fights  (designated  In  tables  as  r&t  fights;  see 
Table  1).  Within  the  single  condition  males  were  the  more  aggressive 
sex  in  a  significantly  larger  number  of  groups  by  all  measures  except 
the  number  of  rough-and-tumble  fights  and  the  duration  of 
rough-and-tumble  fights.  Within  the  paired  condition  males  were  the 
more  aggressive  sex  In  a  significantly  larger  number  of  groups  for  the 
measures  of  number  of  attacks,  number  of  fights,  duration  of  fights  and 
number  of  approaches.  There  was  no  measure,  for  either  the  paired  or 


42 


Table  1 


Comparison  of  the  Number  of  Groups  In  Which 
Males  or  Females  Were  More  Aggressive 


Total 

(N=20) 

Paired 

(N=10) 

Sing 

e  (N=10) 

Measure 

Mai  e 

Female 

Male 

Female 

Male 

Female 

No.  of  attacks 

19 

1*#* 

9 

1* 

10 

0** 

No.  of  fights 

17 

2*** 

9 

1* 

8 

1* 

Duration  of  fights 

18 

2*** 

9 

1* 

9 

1* 

No.  of  chases 

17 

3** 

8 

2 

8 

1* 

Duration  of  chases 

17 

3** 

8 

2 

9 

1* 

No.  of  r&t  fights 

13 

5 

6 

2 

7 

3 

Duration  of  r&t  fights 

15 

3** 

8 

2 

7 

1 

No.  of  approaches 

19 

1*** 

10 

0** 

9 

1* 

No.  of  displacements 

15 

4* 

7 

3 

8 

1* 

All  durations  are  In  seconds. 

Sign  test     2-tail    *£<.05     **£<.01     ***£<. 001 


43 


single  condition,  for  which  females  were  more  aggressive  than  males  in 
a  larger  number  of  groups. 

Males  In  general  also  exhibited  higher  total  levels  of  aggression 
than  females  by  all  measures  (see  Table  2).  Males,  in  both  single  and 
paired  groups,  exhibited  a  higher  frequency  of  attacks,  fights,  chases, 
displacements  and  approaches  than  females.  They  also  exhibited  longer 
durations  of  chases  and  fights  than  females  In  both  types  of  groups 
(see  Table  3).  Although  differences  in  the  total  amount  of  submissive 
behavior  exhibited  by  males  and  females  across  both  groups  were  not 
statistically  significant  (±=1.28,  d_£=19,  £>.  05),  males  had  more 
submissive  behavior  directed  toward  them  than  did  females 
(pa!red-±=2.71,  d±=19,  £<.05). 

Because  aggressive  digging  was  not  immediately  recognized  as  a 
possible  correlate  of  aggression,  it  was  not  recorded  for  the  three 
Initial  paired  groups.   It  was  recorded  for  all  subsequent  paired  and 
all  single  groups.  No  significant  differences  in  the  frequency, 
duration,  or  mean  duration  of  aggressive  digging  were  apparent  in 
comparisons  between  paired  and  single  animals.  Males  displayed  higher 
levels  on  all  of  these  measures  than  did  females,  with  the  exception  of 
the  comparison  of  the  average  duration  of  aggressive  digging  for  single 
animals.  High-aggression  males  (those  In  each  group  with  the  highest 
total  frequency  of  aggressive  behavior)  displayed  a  higher  frequency  of 
aggressive  digging  than  low-aggression  males  (paired-±=2.30,  d±=16, 
£<.05).  The  difference  between  high  and  low-aggression  animals  In  the 
level  of  aggressive  digging  they  displayed  may  be  due  In  part  to 
differences  In  the  response  of  these  two  classes  of  individuals  to 


44 


Table  2 

Comparison  of  Total  Aggression  V/ithin  Groups  by  Males 
and  by  Females  In  Both  Paired  and  Single  Conditions 


Mai 

e 

Fema 

e 

Measure 

Mean 

(SE) 

Mean 

(SE) 

t 

No.  of  attacks 

46.30 

5.95 

10.20 

2.41 

5.23**** 

No.  of  fights 

14.55 

2.24 

3.05 

1.07 

4.23**** 

Duration  of  fights 

23.00 

3.96 

4.60 

1.46 

4.31 **** 

No.  of  chases 

116.20 

12.18 

21.85 

5.51 

5.98**** 

Duration  of  chase 

679.10 

79.01 

128.40 

32.79 

5.59**** 

No.  of  r&t  fights 

5.00 

1.44 

1.25 

.42 

2.60* 

Duration  of  r&t  fights 

10.30 

3.04 

2.80 

1.39 

2.35* 

No.  of  approaches 

64.10 

7.91 

23.40 

3.61 

5.62**** 

No.  of  displacements 

12.80 

2.59 

3.40 

.90 

3.43*** 

All  durations  are  in  seconds. 
Paired  ±- test    d±=19     2-tail 
****£<. 001 


*£<.05 


*£<.01 


45 


•i-  T3 
(/)     ro 

ai 

s-  -o 
en  aj 
01  s- 

n3 
<+-  Q. 
O 


> 

00 

QJ 

O) 

1 

m 

1 — 

F 

m 

aj 

+j 

u. 

lO^voor-'TOvoo^ooi  —  r»< 


>  r»  o  o  cm  <n  o  o  » 


lh«ONtNONOhh« 


1  O  O*  O  O  P-  CO  «0 

!  *o    "  o^  ■*  o  o 


—  p^Kicor-^ood'or— cm 

ir\  <n  in  r- 

mifio       *o  *o  *r  —       — r* 

«r*irt« 

■.0  0  —  — 

cm        —        m  a*  —       cm 

irtOiftr-tfMAp'ino^'inor 


I    —    f-COCO*OvQG*CMC7A 


\0  •—  Kt         >0  -^  •—         <o( 


o«nir\oor-oo> 


)  .—  0\  —  O  CM  P^ 


.coop-Ki(Nir\r-o*o^,~- 


1  o  co  0  r- 


■  o  «r»  —  o  1 


o-«-        0«»-+-0-*-<d£>0'«- 
O  O  -C  O  -C    41  —  o 

C  C  OIC  OU<flC 

lOCWOC—    O    C     «)    C    v>    O    C 

.  —  o  0)  —  o  •* 00—0—0 


00—       o—       o—       000— 

<o  c         «   c         o  c  « 

oooc&dooooajoooo 


46 


noises  within  the  apparatus.  High-aggression  animals  often 
investigated  noises  made  by  other  Individuals  In  the  apparatus. 
Low-aggression  Individuals  generally  Ignored  these  noises  or  retreated 
from  them.  Low-aggression  individuals  that  attempted  to  dig, 
therefore,  In  effect  increased  the  probability  of  attack  by 
high-aggression  Individuals,  whereas  high  aggression  Individuals  dug 
without  Interference. 

Individuals  directed  more  aggression  toward  same-sex  than 
opposite-sex  individuals  in  both  the  paired  and  single  conditions  (see 
Tables  4  and  5).  Males  in  both  single  and  paired  groups  directed  more 
fights  and  rough-and-tumble  fights,  and  longer  durations  of  these 
behaviors,  against  other  males  than  against  females.  Females  in  both 
single  and  paired  groups  directed  more  attacks  toward  same-sexed  than 
opposite-sexed  Individuals.  Males  and  females  in  both  types  of  groups 
directed  more  chases  toward  same-sex  than  opposite-sex  Individuals. 

Although  many  of  the  differences  In  aggression  between  sexes  were 
significant,  and  many  significant  differences  were  also  found  in  the 
level  of  aggression  directed  at  same  versus  opposite-sexed  Individuals, 
the  overall  levels  of  aggression  displayed  by  animals  in  the  single  and 
paired  conditions  were  very  similar  (means  and  standard  errors  were 
presented  in  Table  3).  Animals  in  the  two  conditions  displayed 
significant  differences  on  only  two  of  the  aggressive  measures:  paired 
animals  displayed  longer  average  durations  cf  fights  with  same-sexed 
Individuals  and  a  greater  number  of  approaches  to  same-sexed 
Individuals  than  did  animals  In  the  single  condition  (±=2.12,  d±=78, 
£<.05;  and  ±=2.10,  d±=78,  £<.05  respectively). 


47 


•  in  (sj  —  \o  oo 


com  —  CN^ommmcrico.—  -<r 


CMCOCNm  —  OvOmmCMmm 


O*  <SJ  CM  — 


*  * 

«   *   * 
■  r-  en  en 

co»-vocorsfO'--  •—  Ovo 

—  fOrOCNCNCM  Kt  m  fO  CM 


m  r-  • 


r-  en  en  ki  — 


*—  00.—  k\ooocnik\k\it>cok% 


<onco(Niooi 


i  o  o  o  en  co 


mCMcocoooomminotn 
^OGooocoor^ooop-^r 

*0  —  —         CftCMro  m  CM 


aiCNjonro^>ovoKi^m 

CM  —  CM  CM  ^f  —  —  — 


moifiooin  —  inmr-oo 

Mnov\OtnO>fVN^ro 


C0(NNO(MChOO>0-WKI 

MOOMONonTcnoMnoo 
<N  fO  ro-  mm  —  m  cm  cm  — 


o  o  o  r-*  -<r 


—       ir\  en  cm 


£3 


—         ,-         to  — 


o  — 

9-  E 


Kimo  —  oomoiocoi 

OO^OCDK1(NO<OCOl 


>oioin'-^MO-w 


=3    — 
—    ID 


C     (D 
O     QJ 


+- 

a> 

<1> 

*-  +- 

(O 

O 

u> 

d>«3 

O 

o 

C  —    l_ 

O  +-           O 

<]> 

O  *- 

I) 

1) 

+-  en  o  +- 

i) 

+-  +-  o 

(-L 

H ra 

10  <*5 

L.     A3 

o   o   ^   a)  o 


o  o 

CD     O     O 


ca  tj| 


to  a)  -H 
i_  •*-    i 

3   —    -O 


IZZOE^OEZOE^^ 


48 


(SI  CM  —  —  —  — 


tnK*aomvoK\coK«\mmoK\ 

NOO-VNVOOOCO- 


iOK>comoo?oiftK\toif>coeo 

r^oo  —  —  (MCOOOO-  CM 


M»OK>OK\0<<OOlOOOO 

co  r*  co  *—  —  inhNm-  cocsi 


*o^-»—  m  co  k\  r^  —  o  *o  r-  *— 

CM  CM  CM  K\  »-  CM  CM  CM 

cm  row  CM 


r*»  . cm  irt  cm 


*-  o  ^r  *o  o  -—  ir»  p»  —  ou*\  — 
wooO'-t'-  o  K\  r-  cm  m  m 

CMCMCM  CO  vO  ^-  —  CM  *— 


—  CM  CM  K\  K»  —  CM  CM  •-  K^ 

■r-CM^T—  (O  »  K\0  •-  —  vO  CM 


O   — 


MO»^r»iooroCft  — 


mCMmOrOO\OCM<—  CMCMO* 

cm  —  m  •—  eo  *o  v>  r-»  **  —  C7»cm 


—  —  .-       *r  p^ 


4*3 


mK\mococoir\omcoOco 
CMir\*»^-coo*^rco«—  ^r  r-  to 

A3 

m  fO  ««*         O*  r-  m  ~-  Kl         K\ 

©     0) 

a  — 

—    (0 

«  2: 

j3 

in 

+- 

!s3 

cr 

M 

-c 

+- 

M 

-C 

+- 

l/> 

c 

0) 

in   <u 

• 

C      IO 

+- 

IU    +- 

ft) 

0 

0 

a 

a    o 

•o 

u    (J 

E  h- 

r 

u 

c: 

10     i0 

0) 

0 

0 

o  — 

X 

L_     Q. 

o 

4- 

(> 

o 

a.  in 

in 

+- 

<*n 

n 

CL  — 

r 

a  -H 

(» 

<> 

-i 

U 

+-    i 

v> 

V) 

—   -a 

+- 

(> 

O    O 

"O 

m    a> 

(0    c 

10 

m 

a 

nj 

O      =!     <1) 

(> 

a) 

0 

rj 

0) 

O    O 

ex  o 

-i 

Z  O  £ 

.£ 

o 

i. 

-; 

o 

i. 

z  z 

49 


Comparisons  of  aggressive  measures  between  the  two  conditions 
within  each  sex  yielded  significant  differences  for  females  only  (means 
and  standard  errors  were  presented  in  Tables  4  and  5).  Paired  females 
exhibited  a  greater  number  of  approaches  to  same-sex  individuals  and  a 
greater  number  of  displacements  of  same-sex  individuals  than  did  single 
females  (±=2.57,  d±=38,  £<.02;  and  ±=2.08,  d±=38,  £<.05, 
respectively) . 

The  various  measures  of  aggression  recorded  tended  to  be 
correlated  with  each  other.  The  total  frequency  of  attacks,  chases, 
fights,  and  rough-and-tumble  fights  were  correlated  within  each  sex  for 
both  paired  and  single  groups,  as  were  the  duration  of  chases,  fights, 
and  rough-and-tumble  fights.  The  frequency  of  approaches  was 
correlated  with  the  number  of  chases  for  males  and  females  in  both 
conditions  and  with  the  number  of  attacks  for  paired  males  and  females 
and  single  males  (see  Table  6).  The  amount  of  submissive  behavior 
directed  toward  single  females  was  correlated  with  the  frequency  and 
duration  of  fights  (Pearson  correlation,  £=.724  and  £=.798, 
respectively,  p.<.001)  and  the  frequency  and  duration  of  rough-and- 
tumble  fights  (Pearson  correlation,  e=-704  and  .800,  respectively, 
£<.001).  Submission  was  also  correlated  with  the  total  frequency  of 
approaches  for  paired  females  and  single  males  (Pearson  correlation, 
£=.444  and  £=.515,  respectively,  £<.05).  Frequency  of  aggressive 
digging  was  correlated  with  the  total  frequency  of  aggressive  behavior 
(combined  frequencies  of  attacks,  fights,  chases,  and  rough-and-tumble 
fights)  for  paired  males  and  females  (£=.638,  £<.05,  and  £=.845,  £<.001 
respectively)  and  single  males  (£=.697,  £<.001).  Duration  of 


50 


t—  ^i-  en  to  ^  m  co  »—  to  *—  Nr-ioi^^Chcot^ 
o-—  mcNr^i^icricMCT\CNCNiho<Nor--r~-cMm 


._  © 

10  — 

in   O) 


*  *   *   * 

*  *   *   * 
******* 


*  *  *  * 

*  *  *  * 

*  *  *  * 

*  *  *  * 


cotOfnr-r^-oococNravotovooococotoc* 


******* 


*  *   *  * 

*  *   *   * 


Kimir\^r«3-csi',3-*3-cr\ior<~\'—  in  r-  k\  m  ro  r-~ 


* 


ui  in 

£  ° 
o  o 


tu  to 

<x>  <x> 

a.  i- 


*  ***** 
******* 
******* 


*  *  *  * 

*  *  *  * 

*  *  *  * 
***** 


*******  *****  *  »  *  * 
inor->oOiO'-Mi'ioicriaioocooMO 
r-r~-tnoocoa\oocN'rcN,^CTivocMvor~-mm 
oomr--r--mr-~vO'*i-cor~r~-r~'3-'3'^i'^or--r~- 


in 

10 

0 

0) 

-t- 

+- 

+- 

10 

to 

tt) 

F 

r- 

JZ 

xz 

c 

+- 

+- 

£ 

C|> 

(1) 

u: 

U) 

10 

(0 

(!) 

c 

c 

() 

U 

U 

t/) 

■ — 

• — 

-H 

<i) 

t- 

U) 

0) 

(1) 

U) 

l/l 

0) 

ia 

10 

ro 

+• 

H- 

s- 

_c 

JC 

(1) 

+■ 

(1) 

r- 

+- 

U> 

t- 

o 

— 

— 

XZ 

Ol 

o 

O 

-c- 

r: 

<1> 

XI 

x: 

(1) 

t_ 

a. 

a. 

a  +- 

+- 

(0 

in  — 

10 

ro 

O) 

o 

O 

rr 

u 

c; 

Q. 

to 

to 

— 

°0 

«0 

0) 

+-    H- 

o 

U) 

ro 

10 

lU 

ro 

Q_ 

1_ 

L. 

(0 

-C 

( 

f"L 

a) 

•+- 

O 

w- 

O 

10 

-a 

a 

(0 

C0-H 

a. 

01 

10 

i_ 

a. 

i_ 

O. 

XJ 

X3 

XJ 

r. 

—  «3 

o 

(U 

+- 

Q. 

</) 

+- 

u. 

t/i 

■o 

■u 

T3 

c 

c: 

c 

(.) 

M-    1_ 

in 

XI 

J- 

oO 

O 

«i 

a. 

c 

c 

c 

ID 

(0 

ro 

o 

L 

«1 

■o 

i_ 

ro 

u 

it) 

ro 

ro 

XI 

X3   "0 

T7 

X3 

to 

to 

U) 

r 

C     C 

c 

r. 

T> 

T3 

X3 

■o 

"O 

"O 

"O 

to 

to 

to 

(1) 

CD 

+- 

ro 

ro   ro 

ro 

ro 

c 

r 

C 

c 

c: 

c 

ti- 

+- 

+■ 

tu 

to 

t/i 

x: 

ro 

ro 

ro 

ro 

ro 

ro 

ro 

XZ 

xz 

jC 

ro 

ro 

CO 

(0 

01    Ifl 

to 

(0 

CT 

CT 

o 

XL 

.c 

* 

.^  j*: 

V 

V 

i/i 

10 

(0 

10 

10 

to 

to 

ro 

o 

o 

m- 

o 

u  o 

<) 

o 

+- 

+- 

+- 

+- 

CD 

CD 

0) 

H- 

*- 

u 

in 

(0    (0 

ro 

hi 

x: 

jC 

r 

c 

t/i 

ID 

U) 

u 

4- 

4- 

+- 

+-  +- 

4- 

+- 

rr 

CO 

O) 

a) 

(U 

ro 

ro 

+- 

+- 

C-L 

o 

o 

U 

+- 

+-  +- 

+- 

+- 

_C 

XZ 

-C 

^ 

<Xl 

O- 

(U 

to  ro 

ro 

(0 

M- 

H- 

H- 

m- 

U 

o 

u 

1_ 

i_ 

<U 

c 

c 

c 

o  o  o 


000000000000000-I--I-+- 

ro   ro  ro 
l_   l_  1_ 

0000000000000003D3 


ro  ci 


ro 

in  o 
Q)      . 

—  v 

u  at 

c  * 

0)  * 


(!) 

0) 

0) 

1_ 

to 

-i- 

(U 

ro 

-O 

in 

cz 

to 

ro 

O 

(= 

o 

+• 

i 

+- 

— 

tM 

u  o 

—  1_  CN 

—  O    II, 
<  O  ~Z\ 


51 


aggressive  digging  was  correlated  with  total  frequency  of  aggressive 
behavior  for  single  males  (£=.527,  £<.05)  and  paired  females  (£=.845, 
£<.001).  An  animal's  weight  did  not  appear  to  be  an  important  factor 
In  aggressive  interactions.  Only  the  correlation  between  weight  and 
the  frequency  of  rough-and-tumble  fights  in  single  females  was 
statistically  significant  (£=.378,  £<.05). 

For  paired  groups  comparisons  were  also  made  between  the  level  of 
aggressive  behavior  that  occurred  while  pairs  were  confined  to  the 
corner  compartments  and  the  level  after  access  to  the  entire  apparatus 
was  al lowed.  Very  few  aggressive  interactions  of  any  type  (chases, 
fights,  attacks,  rough-and-tumble  fights)  were  observed  between  pair 
members  during  the  period  pairs  were  confined  to  the  corner 
compartments  (Mean  total  frequency  of  aggressive  interaction  per  paired 
individual. 20,  range  =  0  -  4.0).  Therefore  only  the  total  frequency 
of  aggressive  behavior,  rather  than  the  frequency  of  behavior  in  each 
category,  was  used  for  comparison. 

The  total  frequency  of  all  aggressive  Interactions  (attacks, 
fights,  rough-and-tumble  fights,  and  chases)  between  pair  members  while 
confined  to  the  corner  compartments  was  not  useful  In  predicting 
frequency  of  later  aggressive  interactions  with  pair  members 
(males:£=.034,  p>.05;  females:  £=.087,  £>.05)  or  total  frequency  of 
aggressive  interactions  (males:  £=.015,  £>.05;  females:  £=.118, 
£>.05). 

Pairing  did,  however,  have  some  effects  on  later  levels  of 
aggression.  Males  in  paired  groups  were  less  aggressive  to  the  females 
with  which  they  had  previously  been  paired  than  to  females  with  which 


52 


they  had  not  been  paired  by  the  measures  of  frequency  and  duration  of 
chases  (one-tal I  paired-±=2.18  and  2.22,  respectively,  d±=19,  £<.05). 
Males  also  exhibited  a  higher  frequency  of  approach  to  females  with 
which  they  had  been  paired  (one-tail  paired-±=1  .78,  d_£=19,  £<.05). 
None  of  these  comparisons  were  significant  for  females  (Number  and 
duration  of  chases,  ±=.38  and  .76  respectively,  approach;  t=1  .08, 
d±=19,  £>.05). 

A  problem  arises  when  one  attempts  to  compare  different  classes  of 
animals  as  to  their  levels  of  aggressive  interactions  with  one  another. 
The  problem  is  that  the  total  amount  of  contact  between  different 
classes  of  animals  may  vary.  For  example,  if  females  tend  to  avoid 
other  animals,  but  males  do  not,  Individuals  would  have  more 
opportunities  to  be  aggressive  to  males  than  to  females.  A  difference, 
therefore,  in  the  level  of  aggression  an  individual  expresses  toward 
Individuals  of  one  class  versus  another  may  reflect  a  true  difference 
in  the  frequency  of  aggression,  or  a  difference  in  the  frequency  of 
access  to  individuals  of  the  two  classes.  One  method  of  gaining  a 
clearer  understanding  of  the  level  of  aggression,  and  differences  in  it 
between  classes  of  individuals,  Is  to  construct  a  scale  or  index  for 
comparisons  which  accounts  for  differences  In  frequency  of  contact. 

An  "aggressive  Index"  was  calculated  for  this  study  by  dividing 
the  total  frequency  of  all  aggressive  encounters  initiated  by  an  animal 
(frequency  of  attacks,  chases,  fights,  and  rough-and-tumble  fights) 
toward  any  other  class  of    Individuals  by  the  total  number  of  contacts 
Initiated  by  that  animal  toward  that  class  of  individuals  (total 
aggressive  encounters  plus  the  frequency  of  approaches  and 


53 


displacements).  Although  there  may  be  qualitative  differences  in 
approaches  which  elicit  displacement  or  submission  and  those  which  do 
not  (for  example  an  aggressive  individual  may  signal  its  status  through 
adopting  a  particular  posture),  no  means  was  available  in  the  present 
study  to  detect  these  cues.  Therefore  displacements  or  approaches  with 
submission  were  classed  as  contacts  without  aggression  for  purposes  of 
constructing  the  index. 

No  significant  differences  were  apparent  between  paired  and  single 
groups,  or  in  comparisons  between  males  or  females  of  these  groups,  in 
the  total  frequency  of  contacts  or  the  frequency  of  contact  with 
same-sexed  or  opposi te-sexed  individuals  (see  Talbe  7).   Single  females 
did,  however,  have  a  significantly  higher  overall  aggressive  index 
(frequency  of  all  aggressive  behaviors  divided  by  frequency  of  all 
contacts)  and  a  higher  aggressive  index  against  opposi te-sexed 
individuals  than  paired  females. 

The  total  frequency  of  contact  was  significantly  higher  for  males 
than  for  females  in  the  paired  and  single  conditions.  Although  the 
overall  aggressive  index  was  significantly  higher  for  paired  males  than 
paired  females,  the  difference  between  single  males  and  females  was  not 
significant  (see  Table  8). 

Paired  males  and  females  and  single  males  all  displayed  a 
significantly  higher  frequency  of  contact  with  same-sexed  than 
opposi te-sexed  individuals.  Whereas  both  paired  males  and  females 
displayed  a  significantly  higher  aggressive  index  against  same-sexed 
than  against  opposi te-sexed  individuals,  this  comparison  was  not 
significant  for  single  males  or  females  (see  Table  9).  Paired  males, 


54 


E  "~ 


x  o 

CD  <_> 
IS) 
I  O) 
■  CD  i— 
+->  Cn 
•i-  C 
(/)  «r- 

o  tn 
a. 

;    O.T3 

i  o  c 
n3 
:  -o 

I    c  T3 
i    rO    CD 


in  cn 

** 

CM 

cs 

>o  r- 
o  o 

o 

«* 

CD  O 

r^ 

HI  CO 

CM 

*~ 

o 

o  in 

-- 

*o  cn 

vo  -3- 

CN 

u-\  K1 

" 

^ 

T  CM 

<U 

CT\  UD 

n 

r-  o 

K> 

"" 

in 

o  m 

cu 

O  "» 

r<-\ 

r-  ki 

fO 

VO  fi 

O  O  «3 

(D   CO  V 


r*  cnj  a* 
r-  o  »* 


—    t 

+-     G 


io  a  o 

+-  6  a. 

o  ra  a. 

I—  w  o 


55 


55 


CM  — 


CN    — 
CM  — 


CO   — 
(SI   CM 


£3 


—    «3 

Ul     CD 

Q.  e 

Q    a 

u. 

CTi   CM 

t_ 

CD   -O 

!D 

CD     CD 

*T    TT 

Z3    — 

O   O* 

> 

C 

(D 

CD 

K1 

O   CO 

CD     II 

10  "Si 


CD    — 
1-     CD 

dt      —  c 


□  cj 

CD 

as 


57 


but  not  paired  females,  exhibited  a  lower  aggressive  index  against  pair 
members  than  against  opposi te-sexed  non-pair  member  (two-tail  t-test, 
d_f=19,  males  and  females  respectively:  t=2.25,  £<.05;  ±=1.41,  £>.05). 

The  frequency  of  nesting  arrangements,  for  days  on  which  complete 
nest  data  were  available,  is  presented  in  Table  10.   It  is  of  interest 
that  two  animals  of  the  same  sex  nested  together  only  once  without  an 
opposi te-sexed  animal  also  present.  Animals  nested  as  two  opposite- 
sexed  pairs  en  over  1/3  of  the  days  for  which  data  were  available  and 
almost  another  1/4  of  the  nesting  arrangements  observed  included  one 
opposi te-sexed  pair. 

Only  data  on  nesting  behavior  for  days  on  which  nesting 
relationships  were  known  for  all  individuals  in  a  group  were  analyzed 
statistically.  Nest  data  were  available  for  all  but  one  paired  and  one 
single  group.   Three  days  of  nest  data  (the  total  possible)  were 
available  for  two  of  these  nine  paired,  and  seven  of  these  nine  single 
groups.  Two  days  of  data  were  available  for  six  paired  and  two  single 
groups,  and  only  one  day  of  data  was  available  for  one  of  the  paired 
groups.  The  mean  number  of  days  of  data  available  for  nine  paired  and 
nine  single  groups  were  2.11  days  and  2.78  days,  respectively. 

The  analyses  of  nesting  behavior  presented  in  the  tables  are  based 
on  the  "IV 1  Icoxon-test"  (Siegel,  1956).   Analysis  of  nesting  behavior  by 
this  test  in  the  present  study  may  give  more  "weight"  to  observations 
from  groups  for  which  more  days  of  data  are  available.  Significant 
comparisons  in  the  tables  that  were  not  also  significant  by  the  "sign- 
test"  (Siegel,  1956)  are  noted  in  text.   Animals  generally  nested  with 
opposi te-sexed  rather  than  same-sexed  animals.  Differences  in  nesting 


58 


Table  10 
Frequency  of  a  I  I  Possible  Nesting  Arrangements 


Paired 

Single 

Total 

Groups 

Groups 

A I  1  four  together 

4 

1 

3 

Two  opposite-sex  pairs 

15 

7 

8 

Two  males,  one  female 

3 

3 

0 

Two  females,  one  male 

6 

4 

2 

Two  males 

0 

0 

0 

Two  f ema 1 es 

1 

1 

0 

One  opposite-sex  pair 

12 

2 

10 

None  together 

3 

1 

2 

Only  days  on  which  all  animals  could  be  accounted  for  are  included 
(Total  number  of  days=44). 


59 


O   (Nl  — 

c*j  m  ^r 

k\  co  ir\ 

csj  — 

•—  K\  Ox 


E  4- 

(Q  — 


C    U  — 

<D  ■*■  O 

3  • 

cr  en  v 


C      C      CI 

—        c 

H O 


(D     <D    — 

L)   cr+-   ro 

C    0)    1    +■ 

0)    U     C     | 

3  •*-    O  — 

o*       c 

a>  x 

U    0>  *- 

1 

fc 

(U 

ID 

X    <D    t_    C 

d)   o  ^  o 

E     O-  C    — 

— 

n 

— 

ro  o.  ii   — 

60 


I—    i£>   1 
VO   -=T  CSI 

CM  —  (N 
CN   T  GO 


lONO 
CM  CN  KS 


r 

0 

* 

in 

o 

(I) 

I 

t;i 

<- 

t- 

o 

o 

X 

en 

<. 

r-  o  co 

O    T    "T 
CN  —  — 


—  —  CN 


(N  CN  CN 


61 


frequency  with  same  and  opposi te-sexed  animals  were  significant  for  all 
comparisons  except  paired  animals  in  general,  and  paired  females  (see  Table 
11).  (Ey  the  sign  test  the  comparison  for  all  females  was  also  non- 
significant, x=10).   In  general  animals  also  nested  with  high-aggression 
rather  than  low-aggression  animals  of  the  opposite  sex  (see  Table  12).  This 
was  true  for  animals  in  paired  groups  and  single  groups,  and  for  males  and 
females.  The  finding  of  non-significance  for  comparisons  within  paired  and 
single  groups,  except  single  females,  may  be  due  to  the  small  number  of  non- 
tied  observations  available  for  comparison.   (By  the  sign  test  comparisons 
for  paired  animals  and  males  in  general  were  also  non-significant,  x=2  for 
both  comparisons).  Total  frequency  of  aggression  was  correlated  with 
frequency  of  nesting  with  opposi te-sexed  individuals  for  paired  males  and 
single  males  (Pearson  correlation,  £=.710,  £<.001  and  £=.519,  £<.05 
respectively)  but  not  for  paired  or  single  females  (r=.066  and  r=.21 9 
respectively,  £>.05). 

Animals  did  not  nest  more  frequently  with  familiar  individuals. 
Comparisons  based  on  all  paired  animals,  males,  or  females  were  all 
non-significant  (Wilcoxon,  N=number  of  non-tied  observations:  all 
animals,  N=24,  z=1.24;  males,  N=10,  £=1.22;  females, 
N=14,  z=.60).   Neither  the  number  of  days  pairs  had  built  nests 
together  during  the  nest  building  stage,  nor  the  average  type  of  nest 
built,  was  correlated  with  nesting  frequency  with  pair  members  (Pearson 
correlation,  nest  days,  r=.395;  nest  type,  r_=.250). 
Discussion 

These  seminatural  observations  provide  evidence  that  aggression  is 
likely  to  be  an  important  factor  in  social  interactions  in  £*. 


62 


pol ionotus.  As  predicted  of  the  behavior  of  monogamous  species 
(Kleiman,  1977)  frequent  aggression  was  displayed  by  both  males  and 
females,  and  the  majority  of  aggressive  behavior  was  directed  against 
same-sexed  Individuals.  The  suggestion  that  females  of  this  species 
are  normally  dominant  over  males  (Smith,  1967)  was  not  supported  by  the 
results  of  the  present  study.  Males  displayed  much  higher  levels  of 
aggression  than  did  females,  and  females  generally  exhibited  very  low 
levels  of  aggression  toward  males.   In  addition,  high-aggression  males 
more  frequently  performed  a  behavior,  aggressive  digging,  that  could 
function  to  display  their  aggressive  status. 

Although  the  frequencies  of  aggressive  behaviors  In  paired  and 
single  groups  were  very  similar,  the  aggressive  index  Indicated  that 
individuals  in  paired  and  single  groups  behaved  differently. 
Individuals  in  single  groups  did  not  appear  to  discriminate  between  the 
targets  of  their  aggression  as  well  as  paired  individuals  did. 
Overall,  the  differences  in  aggressive  behavior  between  paired  and 
single  groups  may  indicate  a  tendency  for  reduced  aggression  toward 
opposite-sexed  Individuals,  especially  pair  members,  In  paired 
Individuals.  This  Is  particularly  true  of  paired  males,  which 
displayed  both  reduced  total  frequencies  of  aggression  and  a  lower 
aggressive  Index,  against  females  with  which  they  had  been  paired  than 
against  those  with  which  they  had  not  been  paired. 

Superior  aggressive  ability  would  appear  to  provide  some  social 
benefits  for  individuals,  as  both  males  and  females  nested  more 
frequently  with  high-aggression  rather  than  low-aggression  Individuals 
of  the  opposite  sex.  On  the  other  hand  familiarity,  although  it 


63 


appeared  to  reduce  aggression  between  pair  members,  did  not  have  a 
significant  effect  on  nesting  behavior.   It  would  appear  from  these 
observations  that,  In  P^  pol Ionotus.  aggressive  ability  may  be  a  more 
potent  factor  In  social  preference  than  In  familiarity.  However, 
because  It  is  unlikely  that  Individuals  under  the  present  conditions 
were  always  able  to  control  who  nested  with  them,  it  is  probably  best 
to  use  caution  In  interpreting  these  results. 

Aggression  and  Familiarity  Preference  Tests 
Introduction 

The  results  of  the  semi  natural  experiments  indicated  that 
aggression  may  be  an  important  factor  in  social  preference  in  Pj, 
pol ionotus.  but  cast  some  doubt  on  the  importance  of  familiarity  In  the 
social  preference  of  this  species.  This  experiment  was  designed  to 
test  preference  based  on  aggression  and  familiarity  In  JL.  pol ionotus 
and  E*.  manlculatus  in  a  more  controlled  manner,  through  the  use  of  a 
preference  apparatus. 

Subjects 

Subjects  were  Individuals  of  two  species  of  muroid  rodents,  P_*. 
pol ionotus  and  JL.  maniculatus.  A  total  of  40  animals  of  each  species 
served  as  subjects  for  aggressive  tests  and  aggression  preference 
tests.  Prior  to  serving  as  subjects,  animals  were  maintained  as 
described  in  the  general  methods  section.  Within  each  species  these 
animals  were  each  assigned  to  one  of  10  groups,  with  two  animals  of 
each  sex  per  group.   In  addition  to  the  criteria  described  In  the 
general  methods  section,  no  individual  within  each  group  could  be 


64 


related  by  more  than  two  common  grandparents  to  any  other  animal  in  the 
group. 

Following  the  aggression  tests  and  aggression  preference  tests  the 
animals  described  above  also  served  as  "stimulus"  animals  for 
familiarity  preference  tests,  while  an  additional  40  animals,  20  of 
each  sex  of  each  species,  served  as  "choice"  animals  for  these  tests. 
Procedure 

Procedures  were  identical  for  each  experimental  group.  Animals 
for  each  group  were  separated  from  litter  mates  and  individually  housed 
in  48  x  27  x  13  cm  clear  plastic  cages.  Peromyscus  man  icu I atus  were 
moved  from  their  colony  room  to  the  £*.  pol ionotus  colony  room.  On  the 
following  day,  within  the  first  1/2  of  the  dark  phase  of  the 
photoperiod,  animals  were  lightly  anesthetized  with  ether  and  marked 
for  identification  by  shaving  them  in  one  of  two  patterns:  either  (1) 
a  band  was  shaved  from  around  the  neck  area,  or  (2)  a  band  was  shaved 
from  around  the  middle  of  the  animals.  One  animal  of  each  sex  was 
shaved  in  each  pattern.  Animals  were  placed  in  48  x  27  x  13  cm  plastic 
cages  modified  (as  previously  described  under  aggression  apparatus)  for 
aggression  testing. 

All  adaptation  and  testing  were  conducted  during  the  dark  portion 
of  the  photoperiod.  Animals  were  adapted  to  the  preference  apparatus 
on  the  two  days  following  the  marking  procedure.  On  the  first  of  these 
two  days  animals  were  adapted  to  the  procedure  that  would  be  used  when 
they  served  as  "choice"  animals. 

Adaptation  to  "choice"  procedures  was  as  fol lows:  the  animal  was 
placed  in  the  start  box  for  5  min,  followed  by  1  hour  free  in  the 


65 


apparatus  without  other  animals  present.  Animals  that  did  not  exit  the 
start  box  within  1  1/2  mln  after  the  door  was  lifted  were  gently 
prodded  with  the  eraser  end  of  a  pencil,  often  simply  lifting  the  lid 
of  the  start  box  slightly  provided  sufficient  stimulus  for  the  animal 
to  leave  the  box.  The  same  procedure  was  fol lowed  during  tests. 

The  day  following  adaptation  to  choice  procedures,  animals  were 
adapted  to  "stimulus"  conditions.  Adaptation  for  stimulus  animals 
consisted  of  being  placed  In  the  stimulus  boxes  at  either  end  of  the 
preference  apparatus  for  1  hour.  Animals  which  were  tested  together  as 
stimulus  animals  for  experimental  tests  were  also  adapted  together. 
During  adaptation  of  stimulus  animals  for  the  aggression  preference 
tests  an  opposi te-sexed  "pretest"  animal  was  allowed  free  in  the 
apparatus  and  its  visits  to  either  chamber  recorded.  This  period  was 
designated  as  the  pretest  period.  Each  pretest  choice  animal  had  been 
adapted  to  the  apparatus  previously,  and  served  In  several  pretests 
with  animals  of  the  opposite  sex. 

Following  adaptation  to  the  preference  apparatus  animals  were 
adapted  to  the  "aggression  apparatus"  for  2  hours  on  each  of  the  next 
three  consecutive  days.  For  these  adaptation  periods  the  animals  home 
cage  was  connected  to  the  aggression  arena  by  means  of  lengths  of  Tygon 
tubing  inserted  into  the  holes  cut  in  the  sides  of  the  home  cage  and 
Into  the  sides  of  the  aggression  arena.  Animals  were  restricted  to  the 
arena  for  the  first  40  mln  of  the  2  hr  period  by  means  of  #6  black 
rubber  stoppers  inserted  In  the  ends  of  the  connecting  tubes.  The 
stoppers  were  removed  for  the  remainder  of  the  period  so  that  the 
animal  had  access  to  both  the  arena  and  its  home  cage. 


66 


Aggression  tests  were  conducted  on  3  consecutive  days  following 
adaptation  to  the  aggression  apparatus.  Males  and  females  were 
observed.  Tests  were  conducted  by  placing  the  two  shaved  animals  of 
the  same  sex  into  the  aggression  arena  and  observing  them  for  40  min. 
Behavior  during  this  period  was  categorized  as  approach,  displacement, 
aggressive,  or  submissive.  Frequency  of  all  aggressive  behaviors 
(total  of  all  attacks,  chases,  and  fights)  was  Included  under  one 
category  because  aggressive  behaviors  other  than  attacks  were  extremely 
infrequent. 

Aggressive  preference  tests  were  conducted  on  the  2  days  following 
aggression  tests.  Two  animals  of  one  sex,  that  had  been  tested 
together  in  the  aggression  tests,  each  served  once  as  choice  animals. 
The  two  animals  of  the  opposite  sex,  that  had  been  tested  together  on 
the  aggression  test,  served  as  stimulus  animals  for  both  tests.  Tests 
were  arranged  by  placing  one  of  the  same-sexed  pair  of  stimulus  animals 
In  each  of  the  boxes  at  the  ends  of  the  preference  apparatus  15  min 
prior  to  the  beginning  of  the  test,  and  the  choice  animal  in  the  start 
box  10  min  later.  Tests  were  initiated  by  raising  the  door  to  the 
start  box,  and  thereby  allowing  the  choice  animal  access  to  the 
apparatus.  Test  duration  was  1  hr,  timed  from  when  the  choice  animal 
exited  the  start  box.  Order  of  testing  was  counterbalanced  for  sexes 
across  days.  Although  the  stage  of  estrus  was  not  controlled  for  In 
these  tests,  smears  were  taken  for  each  female  after  she  had  served  as 
a  stimulus  animal  and  on  the  following  day. 

On  the  day  after  the  conclusion  of  aggression  tests  for  a  group, 
the  members  of  the  group  were  each  housed  in  a  29  x  19  x  13  cm  plastic 


67 


cage  with  an  Individual  of  the  opposite  sex.  These  "new"  opposite- 
sexed  individuals  had  been  adapted  on  the  previous  day  to  the  apparatus 
under  the  procedures  described  for  choice  animals.  These  animals  each 
served  once  as  choice  animals  in  the  familiarity  preference  tests  that 
followed.  Animals  that  had  been  paired  for  the  aggression  preference 
tests  also  served  together  as  stimulus  animals  for  the  familiarity 
preference  tests  (The  two  same-sexed  stimulus  animals  for  each  of  these 
tests  were  two  Individuals  that  had  been  partners  In  tests  for 
aggression).  This  arrangement  produced  four  tests  for  each  group,  two 
tests  for  each  same-sexed  set  of  stimulus  animals. 

Smears  were  obtained  from  the  females  of  each  group  prior  to  the 
onset  of  the  dark  period  on  the  seventh  day  after  animals  had  been 
paired.  Tests  were  conducted  with  stimulus  females  only  when  both  of 
these  females  exhibited  smears  consisting  of  at  least  15%    leucocytes. 
This  type  of  smear  would  normally  indicate  a  nonreceptive  state. 
Females  that  displayed  sperm  on  the  smear  were  tested  after  they 
displayed  this  type  of  smear.  Choice  females  were  tested  individually 
if  they  displayed  smears  with  at  least  15%    leucocytes.  Test  procedures 
for  choice  and  stimulus  animals  were  as  described  previously. 
Results 

Aggression  Tests.  The  level  of  aggression  (attacks,  chases, 
fights,  and  rough-and-tumble  fights)  displayed  in  the  aggression  tests 
were  very  low.  No  aggressive  behaviors  were  displayed  in  four  of  the 
ten  groups  by  females  of  either  species.  Peromyscus  pp|  ionptus.  males 
did  not  display  aggression  In  two  groups,  while  P^  m.a,nicu  latus  males 
did  not  display  aggression  In  three  groups.  The  comparison  of  total 


68 


values  (for  all  three  tests)  for  the  four  behavioral  categories 
recorded  are  displayed  by  sex  for  each  species  in  Table  13.  Only  the 
comparison  of  E_l   pol ionotus  males  and  females  on  frequency  of 
aggressive  behavior  was  significant,  with  males  displaying  a  higher 
frequency  of  aggressive  behaviors. 

Because  the  frequency  of  aggression  in  these  tests  was  very  low 
it  was  difficult  to  determine  which  animal  of  a  pair  was  actually  the 
"most  aggressive".   Instead,  the  total  frequency  of  approaches  and 
aggressive  behaviors  was  used  to  provide  an  indication  of  which  animal 
of  a  pair  might  be  more  aggressive.  This  total  frequency  score, 
although  not  an  aggression  score  per  se,  does  allow  a  comparison  of  the 
tendency  to  initiate  Interactions  or  "assert iveness"  of  the  two 
individuals  In  a  pair.   It  would  seem  reasonable  to  expect  that  the 
less  timid  of  two  Individuals  under  these  test  conditions  might  also  be 
more  likely  to  exhibit  more  aggression  under  other  conditions.  The 
individual  of  a  pair  of  animals  in  aggression  tests  that  displayed  the 
greatest  tendency  to  initiate  interactions  will  be  termed  the 
"high- interaction"  individual,  while  the  Individual  that  displayed  the 
lower  tendency  to  interact  will  be  termed  the  "low- interact  I  on" 
Individual . 

Aggression  Preference.  Table  14  presents  data  on  the  preferences 
of  animals  of  each  sex  for  high  or  low- Interact  I  on  Individuals  of  the 
opposite-sex.  No  preference  for  high- interact  ion  animals  of  the 
opposite  sex  was  displayed  by  ELm.   pol  ionotus  of  either  sex.  Perpmyscus 
maniculatus  males,  however,  did  display  significantly  longer  durations 
of  visits  with  high- interaction  females  than  low- Interaction  females 


69 


\a  *r  •—  o 


—  (\i  —  — 


^omin 


r-  ro  co  N 

CN  —  CM  CM 


C  <1)    C 

O  E    O 

j-  _  to  _ 

O    ul  U     l/l 

ro   ui  ro   ui 

O   o 


70 


C     c/1 
>— i    O 

i    a. 


o 

4-  4- 


ro  n3 
E  E 
CD   -f- 


m  ^r  pn 
iONr» 


lOKIlO 

m  m  r— 

in  co  r- 

CN  *r  <N 

m 

—  o»  o 
o  t  r~ 

m  —  en 
^-  vo  — 

"»  K\  CN 

vo  co  en 

> 

> 

>  — 
> 

o 

o 

°o 

in 

-t 

+- 

ra  +- 

3   i_ 

■o   u 

■a 

L 

10    c 

o  o 

-? 

e 

o 

71 


and  16  of  20  males  visited  high- interaction  more  frequently  than 
low- Interact  ion  females  (sign  test,  M=20,  x.=4,  £<.01). 
High- interaction  P.  maniculatus  females  and  low- Interact  ion  P. 
maniculatus  males  both  displayed  significantly  longer  durations  of 
visits  with  high  rather  than  low- interact  ion  animals  of  the  opposite 
sex  (means  for  high- interaction  females  with  high  and  low- Interact  ion 
animals  =  1155  sec  and  159  sec,  one-tall  paired-!:  d±=8,  ±=2.37,  means 
for  low- interact  ion  males  with  high- interact  ion  and  low- interact  ion 
animals  =  1559  sec  and  468  sec,  one-tail  paired-!:  df=9,  ±=2.13; 
£<.05  for  both  comparisons). 

Although  the  stage  of  estrus  for  females  in  aggression  tests  was 
not  controlled,  data  were  available  for  this  factor.  Comparisons  of 
male  preference  for  high- interaction  and  low- Interact  ion  females  were 
made  for  these  tests  in  which  both  stimulus  individuals  were  In 
diestrus  (see  Table  15).  The  only  significant  finding  was  a  preference 
by  Ej.  maniculatus  males  for  high- Interact  ion  females  by  the  measure  of 
duration  of  visits.  This  is  also  the  only  comparison  which  had  been 
significant  when  data  from  all  females  was  included.  Comparisons  could 
not  be  performed  within  the  non-diestrous  condition  as  there  were  no 
cases  for  P_».  pol  ionotus  in  which  both  stimulus  females  were 
non-dietrous,  and  only  two  such  cases  for  £».  manicu latus  females. 

Comparisons  of  male  preference  for  dlestrous  versus  non-diestrous 
high- interact  ion  females  and  diestrous  versus  non-diestrous  low- 
interaction  females  are  presented  In  Table  16.  Peromyscus  pol Ionotus 
males  displayed  more  visits  to  non-diestrous  high- interact  ion  females 
than  to  diestrous  high- interaction  females,  and  Pj_   manicu I atus  males 


72 


Table  15 


Comparison  of  Male  Preference  for 

High- Interact  ion  and  Low- I nteraction 

Females  in  Diestrus 


High  Low 

Interaction      Interaction 


70.86 

11  .41 

76.00 

16.18 

.29 

618.14 

152.07 

419.60 

115.14 

1.33 

11.22 

2.75 

8.47 

2.54 

1.12 

Measure       Mean (SE)    Mean (SE) 

P.  pol ionotus 

No.  of  visits 

Total  duration  of  visits 

Mean  duration  of  visits 


E*.  maniculatus 

No.  of  visits 

Total  duration  of  visits 

Mean  duration  of  visits 


217 

.00 

187.73 

17.50 

3.85 

1.07 

1291 

.28 

510.14 

114.94 

61.94 

2.43* 

71 

.55 

48.03 

6.32 

2.36 

1.37 

All  durations  are  in  seconds. 

Paired-!    Em.   pol  ionotus  d_£=13     E*.  manjcu  I  atus  jH=5 

1-tail       *£<.05 


73 


a\  in  o  cm  m  co 


»—  r»  *o  o  h*  o 
r-  •—  co  cm  -^  r— 


co  vO  oo  \o  co  vo 


in  r^  *o  eft  co  i 


r*  o  vn  o^  ^r  ' 


tlO>OvO' 


|*»  iOOi  o 

m  r- 

CM.  CM  cm  *r 
—          »0  CM 

co  m 
CM  — 

O  CM  O  CM 

O  CM 

Kt  in  *-  o  m  vo 
m  \o  kv  cm  r-  •«* 

* 

cni  in  co  o  —  c* 

^mor^  t  in 


co  m  C7»  10  co  i 


s_  -C    CD 

O     OVi- 
C(-  ■,-  O 


<U  LO  r— 

<+-  d)  ro 

0)  r-  £ 

S_  <T3  <D 


O  O  O  o  vo  in 
m  in  o  o  tj-  o 


—  ■*  r-  o  m  *r 


—  —  m  CM 


— 

«- 

K} 

m 

CM 

o 

- 

>£> 

CO 

o  m  cm 

CM 

*o  CO 

i£>  CO 

•0 

CO 

K1 

in 

r- 

CM 

CM 

VO 

ro  »n  o  o  cm  r- 
r~  0*  r*  o*  "~ 


-  o  oi: 
jz ( 

o  o 


oi  *  —  —  m  <n 

—  o   >  > 

.c  —  >   > 

O    O    O  O  t-  »- 

+-  +•  o  o 

c  c 

in  (a  o  o  c  c 

■t-  H O    O 

—  —  +-  H 

<n   in   ro  ro  -*-  +- 

—  —  c  u  id  ro 

>     >     3  3  l_    l_ 


o  o 

ro   ro   c 

O   O  O   O  <u 

z  z  t-  >-  z  ; 


74 


•r-O.1 


.,— 

1/) 

re 

13 

O 

•>- 

S- 

o 

4-> 

14- 

</l 

aj 

aj 

0 

a 

c; 

1 

ai 

c 

<- 

0 

1001  n  r*  <oo\ 
©  d  o>  ffl  in  r» 


<o  in  co  «or*r» 


o\  w  co  r^  00  ts 
^r  »o  —  o*  Csj 


p-  vo  ro  in  cn  — 


cn  r-»  Oi  p-  *o  ca 
co  oa  r-  o*  m 


0 
0 

O 

CM 

m 

«3  r» 
0  ^ 

k%  m  0 

CO    CO    CM 

CO    —    — 

CM 

^ 

__ 

3 

"~ 

*~ 

~ 

JJ  3 

00  <o 

c* 

CM  U5  ^ 
CO  CO  O 

m  0 
r~  0 

CTi  in  in  m 

CM  |£> 
lO  CM 

0  0  <o  <o 
vo  0  •—  r* 

—  CM 

in  ^r  — 

to  <o  o  *o  o  to 


!-v  -or  to    CD  to 


N£OCONO»N 


O   CM 

r- 

0 

—  m 

CO  o> 

CM  — 
CM  CM 

00  r~ 

CM  — 

0  m  0  0 
»—  m  m  — 

—    O    o»  X 

.e o 

x:  — 
O  o 
■»-  +-  o  o 


-2>5 


o>  *  —  — 


o  o   o   o  •*-  •«- 

+-  +■  00 

c   c 


o  — 

1.  o 
a  a 


in  in  a  q  4-  -1- 

—  —  t_  u   to  10 

>     >      3      3      l_  L_ 

"a  13 

to  c  c 

O    O    O    O    Q>  Q> 


o  o 


75 


visited  non-dlestrous  low- interact  ion  females  more  frequently  than 
diestrous  low- Interact  I  on  females.  All  other  comparisons  of  male 
preference  based  on  stage  of  estrus  were  non-significant.  The  stage  of 
estrus  did  not  significantly  affect  female  preference  for  high  or 
low- interact  ion  males  of  either  species  (see  Table  17). 

Fami I iarity  Preference.  Male  and  female  P.  pol  ionotus  did  not 
display  preference  for  familiar  individuals  over  unfamiliar  Individuals 
of  the  opposite  sex  by  any  measure,  although  the  comparison  of  the 
total  duration  of  visits  did  approach  significance  for  females 
(one-tall  paired-±,  di=19,  ±=1.47,  £=.054).  Peromyscus  maniculatus 
males  and  females  displayed  significantly  more  visits  to  familiar  than 
to  unfamiliar  individuals  of  the  opposite  sex.  Females  also  displayed 
longer  durations  of  visits  to  familiar  males  than  to  unfamiliar  males 
(see  Table  18),  and  a  greater  number  of  females  exhibited  more  visits 
to  familiar  than  to  unfamiliar  males  (one-tail  sign  test,  N=18,  21=5, 

H<.05). 

Because  stimulus  animals  in  familiarity  preference  tests  had 
previously  been  evaluated  In  aggression  tests,  the  "attractiveness"  of 
these  animals  In  familiarity  preference  tests  could  also  be  evaluated 
on  the  basis  of  their  tendency  to  interact  with  other  animals.  These 
comparisons  are  presented  in  Table  19.  Peromyscus  pol ionotus  females 
displayed  significantly  more  visits  to  high- interaction  than  to 
low- Interact  ion  males,  and  the  difference  In  the  number  of  females  that 
spent  longer  durations  with  high- interact  ion  than  low- Interact  ion  males 
was  also  significant  (one-tai I  sign  test,  N=20,  x=5,  £<.05). 


76 


■  r- 

o 

c 

rn 

<c 

UJ 

S- 

M- 

n3 

O 

00 

<D 

F 

I — 

fO 

A3 

■«r  o  I 


CM  00  lO 

T  o  ■»■ 

CM  CM  — 

«-  >o  o 
m  o>  r» 


r»  ao  »© 

»-  C7»  CM 

—  00  Ol 

<7>  csj 

oo  o  m 

—  "*    «T 

m 

o  o  in 
o>  in  \o 

Nirno 

ro  oo 

CM  00  CO 

\0     .     . 
•  mm 

cm  r-  — 
o  o\  o 


o  — 

.  +-     <H 
O    O     0) 


to  — 

>  — 

e 
woe 

H O 

—  -I 

Ul    <o  +- 

—  i_   io 
O  — 

.  +-     10 

O  O   a 


in 

c 

O   — 

n 
c 
o 

(O  +H 

i_    i 

■o    0) 


77 


TJ 

03 

C 

— 

in 

>i 

C 

4-> 

o 

s- 

4-J 

ro 

o 

•i— 

m 

S- 

aj 

Y- 

+-> 

tO 

a. 

U- 

ai 

00 

m 

m 

y 

E 

>> 

c 

-O  <C 

noo> 

K\  r—  co 
—  rr,  irv 

M  K>  l"1 

l\K\N 

co  N  m 

k\  co  r- 


C>  O  «C 

0>ON 


*«•  ^  CO 

IONO 

CM  K>  «3 

O  — 


—   M 


i/i 


>  — 
> 

e 
o  c 


©  .- 
i_  ■ 

"3 


o  — 

(0  c 

t   +-  <0 

O   o  a> 


78 


Discussion 

Whereas  individuals  observed  in  the  seminatural  apparatus  had 
exhibited  appreciable  levels  of  aggressive  behavior,  levels  of 
aggression  displayed  by  individuals  in  the  aggression  apparatus  were 
very  low.  This  finding  was  somewhat  unexpected.  The  object  of  the 
procedures  followed  in  the  present  study  had  been  to  condition 
Individuals  to  treat  the  aggression  arena  as  an  extension  of  the  home 
cage.   In  previous  observations  Individuals  housed  in  cages  Identical 
to  those  used  to  form  the  aggression  arena  had  displayed  fairly  high 
levels  of  aggression  to  Intruders  after  one  to  two  weeks  of  residency 
(unpublished  observations).   In  the  present  study  Individuals  were 
tested  for  aggresslvity  in  the  arena  on  the  seventh  through  ninth  days 
of  residency  in  the  home  cage.  Although  individuals  did  not  have 
access  to  the  home  cage  during  aggression  tests,  they  had  been  allowed 
to  travel  freely  between  the  home  cage  and  aggression  arena  during 
adaptation.   In  addition,  during  aggression  tests  the  home  cages  and 
the  aggression  arena  were  attached  in  a  manner  that  allowed  a  fairly 
free  flow  of  air  between  them.  Many  Individuals  were  observed  to  spend 
long  periods  sniffing  and  gnawing  at  the  entrances  to  their  home  cages. 
Individuals  therefore  appeared  to  recognize  their  home  cage  as  opposed 
to  a  strange  cage  (unfortunately  these  observations  were  not 
quantified).  During  aggression  testing,  however,  individuals  did  not 
behave  as  residents  in  the  aggression  arena,  nor  did  they  defend  the 
entrances  to  their  home  cage.  Animals  In  these  tests  were 
simultaneously  exposed  to  olfactory  cues  from  both  their  home  cage  and 
that  of  their  opponent.  These  test  conditions  may  have  led  to 


79 


conflicting  "fight"  and  "flight"  responses  (See  Hinde,  1966),  and 
thereby  resulted  in  low  aggression  scores. 

It  is  of  interest  that  individuals  In  the  seminatural  apparatus 
displayed  high  levels  of  aggression  toward  one  another  Immediately  upon 
being  allowed  access  to  the  central  arena,  even  though  none  of  these 
Individuals  had  previous  exposure  to  this  area,  and  had  been  in 
residence  In  the  connected  home  areas  only  three  days.  The  observation 
of  higher  levels  of  aggression  under  the  seminatural  conditions  than 
under  aggress  ion- test  conditions  may  be  due  in  part  to  the  fact  that 
individuals  in  the  seminatural  apparatus  were  exposed  to  opposi te-sexed 
individuals  during  tests  while  individuals  in  the  aggression  apparatus 
were  not  (Barnett,  Evans  &  Stoddart,  1968;  Brain,  Benton,  &  Bolton, 
1978;  deCantazaro,  1981;  Flannel ly  &  Lore,  1977;  O'Donnell, 
Blanchard,  &  Blanchard,  1981).  Exposure  to  females  has  been 
demonstrated  to  Increase  male-male  aggression  In  Ejl   rngpiculatus  balrdl 
(Termen,  1982;  Dewsbury,  Personal  communication).  Exposure  to 
opposi te-sexed  Individuals  was,  however,  not  required  to  elicit 
aggression  in  the  previously  mentioned  tests  of  resident  £*.   pollonotus 
In  aggression-arena-sized  cages;  and  males  in  the  seminatural 
single-housed  condition  were  observed  in  several  tests  to  Initiate  high 
levels  of  attacks  and  chases  prior  to  exposure  to  females. 

Preferences  for  high-Interaction  (and  presumably  more  aggressive) 
individuals  were  not  as  strong  as  might  have  been  predicted  from  the 
results  of  the  seminatural  experiments  in  the  present  study,  or  from 
the  results  of  previous  studies  (e.g.,  Blair  &  Howard,  1944; 
Eisenburg,  1962).  Although  in  the  majority  of  comparisons  (over  80?) 


80 


scores  on  the  measures  of  preference  for  high- interaction  individuals 
were  higher  than  those  for  low- interact  ion  individuals,  most  of  the 
differences  displayed  between  high  and  low- interact  I  on  individuals  were 
non-significant.  This  may  be  a  reflection  of  the  low  levels  of 
aggression  displayed  In  these  tests.  Previous  Investigators  (e.g. 
Huck  et  a  I.,  1981)  have  hypothesized  that  the  differences  In  odors 
displayed  by  dominant  and  subordinate  animals  are  mediated  by 
differences  in  the  physiological  changes  induced  In  these  Individuals 
through  aggressive  encounters.  The  levels  of  aggression  in  the  present 
tests  may  not  have  been  high  enough  to  fully  induce  the  physiological 
changes  necessary  for  clear-cut  discriminations  on  the  part  of  choice 
animals. 

Although  levels  of  aggression  were  low  in  aggression  tests,  females 
of  both  species,  and  E*.  manlculatus  males,  displayed  significant 
preferences  for  the  more  "assertive"  individual  of  the  opposite  sex 
under  some  preference  test  conditions.   Significant  preferences  were 
not  displayed  for  low- interact  I  on  individuals  of  either  sex  in  either 
species.  Under  the  assumption  that  Individuals  that  are  more  assertive 
would  also  normally  be  more  aggressive,  these  findings  are  consistent 
with  the  hypothesis  proposed  earlier  that  Individuals  of  these  species 
should  prefer  aggressive  opposite-sexed  individuals,  and  also  with  the 
nesting  behavior  exhibited  by  EU.  pol ionotus  In  the  semlnatural 
apparatus. 

In  familiarity  preference  tests  JL_  manlculatus  of  both  sexes 
displayed  preference  for  familiar  Individuals  of  the  opposite  sex, 
while  Ex  pol ionotus  did  not  display  such  preference.  Although  the 


81 


lack  of  preference  displayed  by  £.  pol ionotus  In  the  familiarity 
preference  tests  Is  consistent  with  observations  made  In  the 
seminatural  study,  results  of  the  familiarity  preference  tests  are 
contrary  to  predictions  made  earlier  as  to  the  behavior  of  these  two 
species. 

Sibling  Preference  Tests 
Introduction 

Individuals  of  most  rodent  species  will  be  exposed  to  siblings 
during  development.  Such  exposure  may,  of  itself  or  in  conjunction 
with  genetic  factors,  influence  mate  selection  (Grau,  1982;  Halpin  & 
Hoffman,  1982;  Smith,  1966).  The  general  consensus  holds  that,  except 
under  special  circumstances,  Individuals  should  prefer  to  breed  with 
non-relatives  (Daly  &  Wilson,  1978;  Dewsbury,  1982a;  Krebs  &  Davles, 
1981;  WIttenberger,  1981).  Rasmussen  (1970),  however,  has  suggested 
that  Inbreeding  may  be  fairly  extensive  In  some  species  of  Peromyscus. 
particularly  JPj.  maniculatus  (Rasmussen,  1964);  and  Howard  (1949)  has 
proposed  that  inbreeding  may  account  for  as  much  as  10  percent  of 
breeding  in  P.  maniculatus.  Smith  (1966)  has  suggested  that  "a 
considerable  amount  of  Inbreeding"  (p.  50)  also  occurs  In  Pj. 
pol ionotus. 

Not  all  Investigators  agree  with  these  proposals  however. 
Selander  (1970)  has  questioned  the  genetic  basis  for  Rasmussen' s  (1964) 
conclusions  regarding  P_».  maniculatus.  and  other  Investigators 
(Dewsbury,  1982a;  Hill,  1974)  have  demonstrated  suppressed 
reproduction  for  sibling  matings  In  this  species.  Foltz  (1981b)  has 
also  questioned  Smith's  (1966)  proposal  of  high  levels  of  Inbreeding  In 


82 


EL.  pol ionotus.  The  present  study  was  designed  to  evaluate  preference 

for  sibl  Ings  in  P^  pol  ionotus  aM  EL  manlculatus. 

Subjects 

Both  FLl  pol Ionotus  and  EL  manlculatus  served  as  subjects  for 
sibling  preference  tests.  Subjects  were  selected  from  12  litters  of 
each  species  that  contained  at  least  two  animals  of  each  sex.  Two 
animals  of  each  sex  were  selected  from  these  litters,  under  criteria  in 
the  general  methods  section,  and  maintained  together  throughout  the 
experimental  procedure.  Litters  selected  for  groups  were  unrelated  by 
more  than  one  common  grandparent.  Two  I  itters  of  each  species 
comprised  an  experimental  group. 
Procedure 

Individuals  were  ear-punched  for  identification,  and  EL 
manlculatus  I itters  were  moved  to  the  EL  pol ionotus  colony  room.  On 
the  following  day  animals  were  adapted  to  the  preference  apparatus, 
without  other  individuals  present,  as  both  stimulus  animals  and  choice 
animals. 

Preference  tests  were  of  two  types,  same-sex  tests  and  opposite- 
sex  tests.   In  same-sex  tests  choice  animals  had  a  choice  between  a 
same-sexed  sibling  stimulus  animal  and  a  same-sexed  nonslbling.   In 
opposite-sex  tests  animals  had  a  choice  between  two  opposlte-sexed 
individuals,  one  a  sibling  and  one  a  nonslbling.  Each  animal  in  a 
group  served  twice  as  a  stimulus  animal  and  once  as  a  choice  animal  for 
each  type  of  test.   In  an  opposite-sex  test,  for  example,  males  2  and  4 
would  serve  as  stimulus  animals  for  choice  females  2  (one  of  the  2  male 
siblings)  and  4  (a  4  male  sibling).  They  would  each  also  serve  as 


83 


choice  animals  with  stimulus  females  1  (a  2  male  sibling)  and  3  (a  4 
male  sib  I Ing). 

Preference  tests  were  conducted  on  the  two  days  following 
adaptation.  All  tests  of  one  type  (i.e.  opposite-sex  tests)  and  half 
of  the  tests  of  the  other  type  were  conducted  on  the  same  day.  Testing 
was  completed  on  the  second  day.  The  order  of  test  type  across  days 
was  counterbalanced.  The  number  and  duration  of  visits  to  each  chamber 
were  recorded  on  each  test  and  for  the  adaptation  period.  Vaginal 
smears  were  obtained  for  each  female  before  the  beginning  of  the  dark 
cycle  on  each  test  day. 
Results 

Peromyscus  pol ionotus  females  demonstrated  a  preference  for 
siblings  over  nonsibllngs  (see  Table  20).  Sibling  males  were  visited 
significantly  more  frequently  than  nonsibl Ing  males,  and  a 
significantly  larger  number  of  females  spent  longer  durations  with 
sibling  rather  than  nonsibl  ing  males  (sign  test,  N=24,  x.=6,  2-tai  I 
£<.05).  They  also  spen+  significantly  longer  durations  with  sibling 
rather  than  nonsibl ing  females.  None  of  the  comparisons  for  siblings 
versus  nonsibl  ings  were  significant  for  P*.  pol  Ionotus  males  or  for  P.^. 
manlculatus  of  either  sex  (see  Tables  20  and  21).   It  Is,  however,  of 
Interest  that  scores  for  £».  pol Ionotus  males  on  tests  with  opposlte- 
sexed  animals  mirror  those  of  JL.  pol ionotus  females  (higher  scores  for 
siblings),  while  for  all  but  one  measure  (average  duration  of  visits 
for  females)  P.,.  manlculatus  display  higher  scores  for  opposl te-sexed 
nonsib I Ings. 


84 


D_  |JD 


c 

-o 

tu 

c 

S- 

rrs 

<D 

H- 

00 

dl 

CD 

S- 

c 

—  o  no 


00  O*  i 
O  I—  ' 


\o  en  CN 

r-ON 
o  en  cn* 


•-  cm  r~ 


*r  to  co 
K>  f>  *r 


O 

CN 

CM 

ir\  csi 

CM 
CO 

CSI  vO 

CN 

o 

CM 

HI 

CO 

CO 

en 

-no 

*-  m  — 

•-  CO 


cm  >r  io 

O  —  T 

—  VO 

O  O  CN 

—  ^-  CO 

Oi  —  r— 


OO  CO 
O  »-  CN 

.-  in 


<o  evi  r- 
o  r—  •» 


0)  —  -I-  — 


CD     C 
L.     C 

djo 


o  — 

a   c 

•  ■*-     IO 

o  o  a 

Zhi 


111   I 


ID   -H 

1-    I 

-O    0) 
—    (0 


85 


s_ 

cu 

o 

■»-> 

4- 

•r— 

1/1 

00 

o 

CI) 

£JL 

a. 

03 

o 

CM  — 

r-  o*  ■— 
io  co  m 

inovai 


co  co  in 
r—  cm  r~ 

■«r  t  ro 
.-  en  — 

it  —  cm 
m  cm 

CM 
r~  VO  CM 

r~-  m  m 

.-  co  io 

co 


m  in  co 


CO   VO  Ot 
O  CM  Oi 


—  •»•  CM 

CM  \0  OI 


_   CO    T 

io  r~  r-* 

•-  m  cm 

oo  — 


>  — 

> 

c 
Ul    o   c 
H O 

—  H 

in    io  +- 

—  i_   <o 
>  3   L 


Kl 

>o 

r- 

m 

m 

CM 

o 

CN 

K1 

O 

r~ 

cm 

CM 

CM 

»0  H"l 
p~  CO 

K\ 

CO 
CM 
CM 

CO 
CO 

—  o 

CO 

^f 

CM 

P-  O   CM 
"~  CM  — 


IIS  Kl  >0 

co  -«t  t 


CM  m  K\ 


\n 


>  — 
> 

o  *- 

o 

c 

O    a 


<*-  TO 

O    — 


S5 
VI    II 

.3 


86 


Table  22 


Comparison  of  Male  Preference  for 
Sibling  and  Nonsibling  Females  in  Diestrus 


Measure 


s 

bl 

ng 

Nons 

ibl  inq 

Mean 

(SE) 

Mean 

(SE) 

t 

90.83 

777.06 

28.66 

24.40 

173.36 

13.16 

95.33 

904.81 

35.11 

21  .10 

259.62 

15.51 

.33 
.41 
.94 

P.  pol ionotus 

No.  of  visits 

Total  duration  of  visits 

Mean  duration  of  visits 


Ej.  maniculatus 

No.  of  visits  18.21  4.45  14.64  3.22  1.36 

Total  duration  of  visits  1048.67  323.90  1448.26  367.33  .64 

Mean  duration  of  visits  87.96  35.79  201.42  84.01  1.12 


All  durations  are  in  seconds. 

±-test     P.  pol ionotus  dl=1 1     P.  maniculatus  df=14     2- tail 

£>.05  for  all  comparisons 


87 


en  c/> 
•r-    o 


en 
>>  c: 

X>  -r- 


c»  k>  m  into  m 

Mnir>coif\  «o 


CM  *r  K\  —  cm  \o 

*—  \Q   CN  CM  ** 


o  o  ^o  vo  o  \o 


CN  CN  P-  CN  vO  •— 
—  CM  O  r*  Kl  CM 


Kt  CN  —  "*  CO  O 

r*  o  cm  10 

CM  f^\ 
00  CD  CO  CO  CQ  00 

CO  ^  00  O*  f-«  Kt 


<f  CD  M  N  CTIVO 

«<r  cm  ■**■  -*r  r-»  in 


<o  o  *o  o  *o  *o 


OK1M  CO  "T- 
inCOCOMOO 


—  CO  CT  CM  T  ■ 


TOO-NO 

oo»coomco 


CO  00  CO  CO  00  00 


kO  r-  Kl  (N  CO  O 
O  VO  CO  o  o  o 


c 

01  — 

C    .O 

J3 

c 

—     Ul 

O  XI    C 

Ul 

C 

+- 

+■   +- 

ifi     Ul 

0 

> 

> 

t/i 

'- 

^_ 

^_ 

>  > 

O 

-t- 

o 
+- 

u 

O 

0  0 

o 

<) 

c    c 

+- 

+- 

<n 

01 

+-  +- 

(0     to 

> 

3 

-1 

O 

r)    a 

■0  "0 

o 

0 

•J 

In 

c    c 

to    (a 

o 

o 

0 

0 

<P    0) 

88 


s_ 

on 

+J 

3 

on 

+J 

01 

ro 

en 

3 

U 

>i 

*f— 

XJ 

c 

m 

en 

£= 

r-O-l 


»—  CO 


s_ 

on 

o 

71 

«4- 

o 

■^ 

O) 

+-> 

o 

on 

c 

o 

O) 

s- 

a 

(11 

i 

M- 

c 

Ol 

n 

i_ 

TZ. 

k\  r- 

** 

CM 

-»    CO 

CM 

CM 

-*  \o 

\£> 

o>  as 

in 

m 

m  in 

\o  in 

CM 

\£)  «D 

<£> 

>o 

lO  *£> 

O   K*l 
O  K1 

\d  if 

'-•-OlOM 


CM  CO  k\  •<*  —  rn 


CO  CO  CO  CO  t 


(OOOm^CO 

m  in  r-  o  —  k\ 

*T  \£>  O  VO  CTt  p*. 

co  en  cm 


r~  o 

r- 

a.  o 

lO 

10  r- 
cm  in 

CM 

CM 

CO  f— 

CO  o> 

in 

>0 

—  o 

CM  CM 

K1 

CO  lO 

o  — 

m 

in  in 

m 

in  in 

in 

o  o 
cm  -=r 

o 

CM 

K1  o 

m  o 

r» 

m  "3-  co  o  rn  r* 
r-  r-  —  r*  cm 


en  vo  \o  cm  o  m 


in  en  r^  ^r  m 

■*• 

\o  co  en  r-  *o 
CM  «—  —  ■** 
CM  — 

t 

en  en  en  en  en 

en 

int\oOT 
en  en  cm  o  en 

in 
tn 

•-       o  in 


O) 


at  —  in  — 
C  J3  o>  — 
—  —    C  .O 

—  10 
_  -Q  C 
c  —    O 

in  u   c 

OlO    o 


-    Ofl 


+■  o  o 


ja  c  —  —  in   in 

—  O    >  > 

in  c  >   > 

O  O    O  O  ■«-  ■»- 

■>-  +-  o  o 

c  c 

in  in   o  O  c   c 

+-  < O   O 

—  —   +-  H 

in  in  ro  ro  +-  +- 


89 


CO 

00 

=s 

V 

o 

1 — 

u 

rn 

+-> 

f= 

LH 

a; 

0) 

u_ 

CDQ-I 


r—  C     3 


a.  | 


s_ 

s_ 

o 

+j 

4- 

00 

C1J 

(!) 

o 

Q 

C 

I 

aj 

C 

s_ 

o 

—  00  —  <N  CM  rr\ 


- 

- 

co 

CM 

to 

CM 

CM 

CM 

in  *r 

to  r- 

cn 

r~ 

CO  — 

o>  — 
m  vo 

in  o 
m  vo 

in 

2 

m 

m 

m  in 

o 

CO 

o 
o 

o 

in 

^r  t 

fO 
CM 

CO 

vo 
in 

CO 
CO 

vo  CM 

en  r~ 

CO 

1— 

CO 

z 

o  vo 

r-  — 

IO 

CM 

CM 
VO 
CM 

to 

T  CO 
VO  Cn 

- 

r- 

r~ 

r> 

r-  r- 

CO  VO 

—  O 

m  o  co 
r-  vo  o 

to 

o 

fO 
CO 
O 

to  vo 

CM  O 

—  to 

r-»  p^  p*-  o  p*-  o 


VO 

Cn  CO 

CO  vo 

to 

CO 

„ 

f 

o 

VO 

— 

o 

IO 

r» 

CM 

in 

to 

CM 

CM 

CM 

in 

CM 

— 

in  m  m  m  t 


O  O  P-  O  (N  I 


p*.  k\  o  p-  co  t 


p*  k\  <7*  —  r-  t 
in  \o  o  o  r-  i 


VO 

to 

to 

to 
o 

CM 

CO 

VO  IO 

cn  cn 

cn 

cn 

cn 

cn 

tO 

vO 

to 

to 

in 

X 

c 

CD  — 

in  — 

C  JO 

—  m 

—  O 

J0  c 

in  c 

in  c 

c  +-  +■ 

—  o   >   >  —  — 


^3 

o 

o  u> 

n>  = 

in  +- 
o 

c  c 

—  o 

ffl  — 


o 

+- 

o 

+- 

o 

o 

r 

O  O 

0 

c  c: 

O  O 

in 

n) 

-I-  +- 

IO  10 

> 

> 

-t 

T) 

-*- 

<a  io 

(1 

a)  at 

^ 

1- 

h- 

E  Z 

90 


As  with  the  aggression  preference  tests  comparisons  were  made  of 
male  preference  for  siblings  and  nonsiblings  in  diestrus  and  of  male 
preference  for  diestrous  versus  non-dlestrous  siblings  and  nonsiblings. 
None  of  these  comparisons  were  significant  for  either  fL.  pol ionotus  or 
fL.  manlculatus  males  (see  Tables  22  and  23).  Comparisons  of  the 
preferences  of  females  in  different  stages  of  estrus  did,  however, 
yield  some  significant  differences.  Although  P^  po| lonotus  females 
exhibited  no  significant  differences  In  preferences  based  on  stage  of 
estrus,  P.  manlculatus  females  did  display  significantly  greater 
preference  for  nonslbling  males,  by  the  measures  of  total  and  average 
durations  of  visits,  when  in  a  non-diestrous  condition  over  a  diestrous 
condition  (see  Tables  24  and  25). 

The  possibility  of  litter  effects  on  preference  scores  was  also 
evaluated  by  conducting  a  one-way  analysis  of  variance  on  the 
"difference  scores"  between  preference  scores  for  siblings  and 
nonsiblings.  Significant  litter  effects  were  found  for  the  average 
duration  of  visits  by  JL.  pol lonotus  males  to  females  (F( 1 1 ,12)=3.77, 
p_<.05).  and  for  the  total  duration  of  visits  by  fL_  pol  lonotus  females 
to  males  (F(1 1 ,12)=3 .08,  £><.05). 
Discussion 

Whi le  Peromyscus  pol ionotus  males  general ly  displayed  higher 
scores  on  preference  measures  for  siblings  than  for  nonsiblings,  these 
comparisons  were  not  significant.  EL.  pol Ionotus  females,  however,  did 
display  significant  preferences  for  siblings  over  nonsiblings.  The 
results  of  this  study,  therefore,  may  offer  some  support  for  proposals 
of  high  levels  of  Inbreeding  In  P.  pol ionotus  (Smith,  1966).  The 


91 


observation  that  scores  for  preference  by  IL.  maniculatus  males  and 
females  were  generally  in  the  opposite  direction  from  those  for  E* 
pol ionotus  Is  of  interest.  This  difference  may  Indicate  a  greater 
preference  for  siblings  by  Em.   pol  Ionotus  than  by  P.  maniculatus. 
It  is  also  of  interest  that  non-diestrous  FL.  maniculatus  females 
displayed  a  greater  preference  than  diestrous  females  for  nonsibl  ing 
males,  since  females  in  a  non-diestrous  condition  would  be  more  likely 
to  be  receptive.  These  results  are  consistent  with  previous  studies 
demonstrating  suppression  of  reproduction  In  sibling  matings  of  E*. 
maniculatus  (Dewsbury,  1982a;  Hill,  1974),  and  the  Implicit  conclusion 
from  these  studies  that  inbreeding  should  be  avoided  by  members  of  this 
species. 


SECTION  IV 
GENERAL  DISCUSSION 

In  the  general  discussion  the  observations  of  the  present  study 
are  examined  in  light  of  previous  research,  and  interpretations  are 
suggested  for  these  observations  that  are  consistent  with  the  ecology 
and  mating  systems  of  Pj.  maniculatus  and  P*.  pol  ionotus.  The  general 
discussion  section  is  divided  into  six  subsections.  The  first  of  these 
subsections  examines  aggressive  ability  as  a  factor  in  preference. 
This  subsection  begins  with  a  fairly  extensive  overview  of  the  ecology 
of  Pj.  maniculatus  and  P_».  pol  ionotus.  and  also  presents  evidence  for 
the  role  of  aggression  in  the  ecology  of  these  two  species.  The 
ecological  information  presented  in  this  subsection  serves  as  a 
background  for  discussions  which  fellow  in  all  subsequent  subsections. 
The  discussion  of  the  ecology  of  these  species  is  followed  by  a 
discussion  of  factors  that  may  provide  an  adaptive  basis  for  selection 
of  aggressive  mates  in  these  species. 

The  second  subsection  deals  with  familiarity  as  a  factor  in 
preference  and  with  the  effect  of  prior  breeding  experience  on 
preference  for  familiar  individuals,  and  includes  a  discussion  of  the 
opportunities  that  may  be  available  for  P_».  maniculatus  and  P,. 
pol ionotus  to  utilize  familiarity  as  a  basis  for  mate  selection.  This 
subsection  is  followed  by  a  subsection  on  the  related  topic  of  kinship 
as  a  factor  in  preference.  The  subsection  on  kinship  discusses 


92 


93 


evidence  for  and  against  Inbreeding  In  JL.  manlculatus  and  EU. 

pol ionotusr  and  the  ecological  and  social  factors  that  may  affect 

preference  for  siblings  as  mates  In  these  species.  This  subsection  is 

followed  by  a  subsection  on  the  evolution  of  monogamy  In  E* 

pol ionotus,  and  the  summary  for  this  study. 

Aggressive  Ability  as  a  Factor  In  Preference 

Many  of  the  observations  In  the  present  study  were  consistent  with 
the  general  hypothesis  that  aggressive  ability  may  be  Important  In  the 
social  behavior  and  mate  selection  of  monogamous  and  polygamous 
species.  They  were  also  consistent  with  the  specific  predictions  that 
aggression  should  be  an  important  component  of  the  social  behavior  of 
E*  pol ionotus,  and  that  P.  pol ionotus  and  E*.  maniculatus  of  both 
sexes  should  prefer  mates  with  high  aggressive  ability.   In  the 
seminatural  experiments  aggression  was  routinely  displayed  by  both 
male  and  female  E*.  pol Ionotus.  and  both  males  and  females  of  this 
species  nested  more  frequently  with  the  more  aggressive  of  the  two 
opposlte-sexed  Individuals.  Female  E*.  pol Ionotus  also  displayed 
preference  for  the  more  assertive  of  two  males  In  familiarity 
preference  tests;  and  preference  for  more  assertive  individuals  was 
displayed  by  E*.  maniculatus  males  and  high- Interaction  P.  manlculatus 
females  In  aggression  preference  tests. 

The  preferences  exhibited  by  these  species  may  be  mediated  through 
advantages  In  reproduction  gained  by  individuals  that  choose  mates  with 
good  aggressive  ability  over  those  that  choose  mates  with  poor 
aggressive  ability.  Some  of  the  possible  advantages  accrued  by 
individuals  that  choose  mates  with  high  aggressive  ability  have  been 


94 


briefly  discussed  In  previous  sections.  The  present  discussion  will 

focus  on  the  advantages  that  might  be  gained  through  such  choice  by 

Individuals  of  the  two  species  of  interest  in  this  study,  P_». 

pol ionotus  and  P^.  manicu latus,  In  the  context  of  the  ecology  and 

mating  system  of  these  two  species. 

Ecology,  Mating  System,  and  Aggressive  Ability 

Many  ecological  variables  will  play  a  role  In  determining  to  what 
extent  aggression  may  be  adaptive  for  individuals  of  any  particular 
species,  and  thus  also  the  Importance  of  aggressive  ability  as  a 
quality  in  mates.  The  ecology  of  a  species  may  often  be  as  major  a 
factor  as  its  mating  system  In  determining  the  Importance  of  aggressive 
ability  to  reproductive  success  in  that  species.  Among  the  more 
general  ecological  problems  with  which  Individuals  of  a  species  must 
cope  with  in  order  to  reproduce  successfully,  and  one  that  also  affects 
the  mating  system  of  a  species,  Is  the  availability  and  defensibi I ity 
of  resources  necessary  to  reproduction  (Brown,  J.  L.,  1975; 
Clutton-Brock  &  Harvey,  1978;  Emlen  &  Oring,  1977;  Hal  I Iday,  1978; 
Orians,  1969).  Aggressive  ability  may  play  an  Important  role  In  the 
acquisition  and  defense  of  these  resources  in  monogamous  and 
non-monogamous  species.   Individuals  that  choose  mates  high  In 
aggressive  ability  may  be  Insuring  that  adequate  resources  will  be 
available  for  themselves  and  their  offspring;  choosing  such  mates 
should  therefore  be  more  adaptive  than  choosing  mates  with  low 
aggressive  abi I ity. 


95 


Peromyscus  manlculatus 

Peromyscus  maniculatus  occurs  In  a  wide  variety  of  habitats  over 
most  of  the  North  American  continent  (Baker,  1968;  Hamilton,  1943; 
Hooper,  1968).  This  species  will  accept  a  wide  variety  of  food  items 
(Cogshall,  1928;  Martel I  &  Macaulay,  1981;  Williams  0.,  1955)  and 
nest  sites  (Blair,  1940;  Hamilton  W.  J.,  1943;  Howard,  1949),  and 
the  distribution  of  the  various  subspecies  in  the  environment  may  be 
governed  more  by  different  behavioral  responses  to  different  habitat 
types  than  by  any  absolute  differences  in  nesting  requirements  or  food 
preference  (Dice,  1922;  Harris,  1952;  Wecker,  1963).  The  subspecies 
of  Ej.  maniculatus  may  for  the  most  part  be  divided  Into  two  general 
types,  those  adapted  forand  occupying  openlands  such  as  fields,  and 
those  adapted  for  and  occupying  woodlands  and  brush  lands  (Baker,  1968). 
Peromyscus  maniculatus  bairdi,  the  subspecies  observed  In  the  present 
study,  Is  generally  found  in  "open  fields,  sand  beaches,  and  arable 
land  of  the  prairie  states..."  (Hamilton  W.  J.,  1943,  p.  270). 

Peromyscus  maniculatus  bairdi  generally  live  in  overlapping  home 
ranges  (Blair,  1940).  As  is  typical  of  other  Peromyscus  (Stickel, 
1968),  each  home  range  contains  several  nests  and  refuge  holes,  and 
mice  may  change  nests  frequently  (Blair,  1940;  Howard,  1949).   In 
addition  to  utilizing  several  types  of  naturally  occurring  nest  sites 
(Blair,  1940;  Howard,  1949)  individuals  of  this  subspecies  are  capable 
of  constructing  their  own  burrows  (Houtcooper,  1972).  Peromyscus 
maniculatus  bairdi  is  therefore  more  likely  to  be  restricted  to 
particular  locations  within  Its  preferred  habitat  by  considerations 
such  as  the  availability  of  sufficient  food  than  by  availability  of 


96 


nest  sites.  This  Interpretation  receives  support  from  observations  by 
Howard  (1949)  which  indicate  that  Increasing  the  number  of  nests 
available  In  an  area  does  not  increase  the  number  of  resident  breeding 
mice  In  that  area;  and  by  observations  which  indicate  that  Pj_  nh. 
bairdl  cache  food  for  winter  use  (Hamilton  W.  J.,  1943;  Howard, 
1949),  and  that  the  availability  of  food  may  limit  the  number  of 
resident  breeding  adults  in  other  subspecies  of  FL.  maniculatus 
(Fordham,  1972;  Gashwiler,  1979;  Taitt,  1981). 

The  habit  of  caching  food  and  the  large  winter  aggregations 
observed  in  this  subspecies  (Howard,  1949,  1951)   are  probably 
adaptations  for  winter  survival.  Energy  requirements  for  these  mice  in 
the  winter  months  are  high  (Howard,  1951)  and  populations  In  some 
localities  may  suffer  severe  winter  mortality  (Blair,  1940;  Howard, 
1949).  These  mice  do  exhibit  an  ability  to  become  torpid  at  low 
temperatures.   In  severely  cold  weather,  however,  individual  mice  may 
not  be  able  to  become  torpid  without  freezing.  Utilization  of  cached 
food  allows  individuals  to  maintain  high  metabolic  rates  (and  therefore 
a  high  body  temperature),  and  huddling  in  winter  aggregations  both 
reduces  body  heat  loss  and  allows  Individuals  to  become  torpid  (Howard, 
1951).  Winter  aggregations  generally  appear  to  consist  of  a  breeding 
pair  and  at  least  one  previous  litter,  additional  conspecific  adults  of 
both  sexes,  and  sometimes  Individuals  of  other  species  (Howard,  1949). 
Peromyscus  maniculatus  bairdi  have  general ly  not  been  observed  to  breed 
In  the  winter  (Blair,  1940;  Howard,  1949),  although  a  few  individuals 
may  sometimes  be  capable  of  breeding  through  the  winter  In 
favorable  microhabltats  (e.g.,  Cornshocks:  Linduska,  1942). 


97 


If  food  availability  is  the  primary  factor  determining  the 
suitability  of  a  particular  area  (within  the  preferred  habitat  type) 
for  breeding  in  P__  m__  bairdi .  one  might  expect  that  it  would  also 
affect  the  social  organization  and  mating  system  of  this  species. 
Because  £__,  nix.  bairdi  appears  to  uti  I  ize  a  wide  variety  of  different 
food  items,  the  Importance  of  which  may  vary  seasonally  (Houtcooper, 
1978),  it  is  likely  that  defense  of  the  food  supply  per  se  Is  not 
economically  feasible.  Peromyscus  maniculatus  bairdi  appear  to  have 
opted  Instead  for  a  strategy  of  limiting  the  number  of  breeding 
individuals  in  or  near  their  home  ranges.  This  Is  accomplished  in  part 
by  adults  aggressively  limiting  juvenile  settlement  in  their  home  range 
(Ayer  &  Whitsett,  1980;  Enders,  1978;  Whitsett,  Gray,  &  Bediz,  1979). 
In  some  subspecies  of  E*.  maniculatus  juvenile  settlement  appears  to  be 
restricted  largely  as  a  result  of  male  aggression  toward  juveniles 
(Fairbairn,  1977;  Healey,  1967;  Metzgar,  1979,  1980;  Mlhok,  1979; 
Sad  lei  r,  1965);  while  for  other  subspecies  (Including  ___.  n__  ba.lr.d_D 
female  aggression  might  be  as  important  as  or  even  more  Important  than, 
male  aggression  In  limiting  juvenile  settlement  (Ayer  _  Whitsett,  1980; 
Enders,  1978;  Fordham,  1971;  Taltt,  1981;  Whitsett  et  al.,  1979). 
As  In  other  subspecies  of  P__.  maniculatus  (Fairbairn,  1978;  Healey, 
1967;  Llewellyn,  1980),  male  aggression  In  P.  m.  bairdi  appears  to 
be  under  hormonal  control  (Whitsett  et  al.,  1979).  The  level  of 
aggression  in  male  £_.  m__  bairdi  may  therefore  follow  a  seasonal 
pattern  of  changes  as  described  for  other  subspecies  of  P__.  maniculatus 
(Healey,  1967;  Llewellyn,  1980;  Metzgar,  1979;  Sadlelr,  1965). 


98 


The  general  seasonal  pattern  of  changes  in  the  level  of  aggression 
for  male  Pj.  maniculatus.  as  described  by  Healey  (1967),  consists  of  a 
spring  increase  In  male  aggression  (which  is  correlated  with  an 
increase  In  testicular  size)  to  a  peak  through  the  major  breeding 
season,  followed  by  a  drop  in  the  level  of  aggression  to  negligible 
levels  at  the  time  fall  aggregations  occur.  This  seasonal  pattern  of 
changes  In  the  level  of  male  aggression  Is  reflected  In  seasonal 
changes  In  social  organization.  At  least  some  subspecies  of  Em. 
maniculatus  have  been  observed  to  form  large  non-aggressive  non- 
breeding  winter  aggregations  (Dice  &  Howard,  1951;  Howard,  1949,  1951; 
Metzgar,  1979).  Although  sufficient  observations  are  not  available  to 
permit  evaluation  of  the  generality  of  the  tendency  of  JL.  maniculatus 
to  form  large  winter  aggregations,  most  subspecies  of  P*.  maniculatus 
do  appear  to  exhibit  a  spring  dispersal  period  prior  to  the  major 
breeding  season.  The  shift  in  behavior  and  social  organization  that 
occurs  at  this  time  have  probably  been  best  described  by  Healey  (1967) 
for  Pj.  nh.   austerus.  According  to  Healey  a  greater  number  of  mice 
survive  the  winter  than  are  compatible  on  the  breeding  territories. 
Animals  which  were  resident  in  the  overwintering  areas,  and  their 
offspring,  are  most  likely  to  be  dominant  In  and  therefore  most  likely 
to  settle  in  these  areas,  while  subordinate  animals  disperse. 
Established  residents  aggressively  exclude  new  settlers,  while 
aggressive  Interactions  between  neighboring  residents  are  reduced.   In 
p.  m.  austerus  mutual  avoidance  between  same-sexed  resident  adults 
may  lead  to  mutal ly  exclusive  home  ranges  within  sexes,  but  overlapping 


99 


home  ranges  between  sexes  (Healey,  1967;  Sadleir,  1965).  Other 

authors  (e.g.,  Metzgar,  1979),  however,  have  observed  a  pattern  In  this 

subspecies  that  is  exhibited  by  several  other  subspecies  of  Ex 

maniculatus  including  Ex  m*.  bairdi  —  same-sexed  home  ranges 

may  overlap,  but  appear  to  overlap  much  less  extensively  than  home 

ranges  for  opposite-sexed  individuals  (Blair,  1940,  1942,  1943;  Howard, 

1949;  Morris,  1955).  The  general  pattern  observed  In  these  studies 

consists  of  large  resident-male  home  ranges  that  overlap  each  other 

slightly,  while  each  also  extensively  overlaps  several  smaller 

resident- female  home  ranges.   In  at  least  some  subspecies  residents  may 

also  tolerate  same-sexed  non-breeding  subordinates  within  their  home 

range  (Metzgar,  1979,  1980). 

The  breeding  system  of  Ex  maniculatus  appears  to  be  somewhat 

labile.  Although  males  generally  appear  to  form  breeding  relationships 

with  a  few  adult  females  within  their  home  range  (Blair,  1958;  Howard, 

1949;  Mihok,  1979;  Terman,  1961),  adults  have  been  observed  in 

combinations  that  included  more  than  one  breeding  individual  of  either 

sex  (Blair,  1958;  Howard,  1949),  and  members  of  this  species  have  also 

been  observed  to  form  long  lasting  and  apparently  exclusive  breeding 

pairs  under  some  conditions  (Blair,  1958;  Howard,  1949).  Metzgar 

(1979)  has  proposed  a  general  system  to  explain  the  lability  apparent 

in  Peromyscus  breeding  systems. 

In  the  proposed  system,  the  home  ranges  of  breeding 
males  overlap  those  of  adult  females  broadly  and  the 
two  classes  occur  together  far  more  frequently  than  by 
chance  alone.  Within  a  sex,  breeding  adults  are  evenly 
dispersed  but  considerable  home  range  overlap  may  occur 
depending  on  density  and  home  range  size.  The  large 
home  range  of  a  breeding  male  usually  includes  all  or 
parts  of  several  adult  female  home  ranges.  Female 
ranges  may  be  overlapped  by  ranges  of  several  breeding 


100 


males,  especially  when  male-male  overlap  is  extensive. 
However,  even  with  extensive  overlap,  each  breeding 
male  might  spend  most  of  his  time  with  a  particular 
female  (Garson,  1975).  Furthermore,  under  some 
conditions  (low  densities  and  small  home  ranges),  this 
generally  loose  male-female  association  might  be 
expressed  as  enduring  male-female  pairs. (p.  142) 

The  particular  breeding  relationships  exhibited  by  a  given 

population  of  E*  maniculatus  may  be  largely  determined  by  food 

availability,  as  food  availability  appears  to  influence  population 

density  and  home  range  overlap  in  this  species,  and  may  also  result  in 

an  alteration  in  the  sex  ratio  of  the  breeding  population.  A  more 

abundant  food  supply  in  an  area  may  result  in  an  Increased  density  of 

adult  mice  in  that  area  (Fordham,  1972;  Gashwiler,  1979;  Taitt, 

1981),  and  a  contraction  of  home  range  for  breeding  individuals  of  both 

sexes  (Taitt,  1981).  Although  Taitt  (1981)  noted  an  increase  in  the 

number  of  both  sexes  in  an  area  supplied  with  additional  food;  Fordham 

(1972)  observed  an  increase  in  the  number  of  females  and  the  proportion 

of  females  breeding,  but  no  increase  in  the  number  of  males.  A 

reasonable  explanation  for  this  difference  was  proposed  by  Taitt  (1981) 

who  noted  that  while  she  had  provided  additional  food  to  a  winter 

population,  Fordham  (1972)  had  provided  additional  food  during  the 

breeding  season.  The  aggressive  resident  males  in  Fordham' s  study  may 

have  prevented  recruitment  of  additional  males,  but  not  females. 

Abundant  food  in  the  breeding  season  may,  therefore,  result  in  a  female 

biased  breeding  population.  A  skewed  sex  ratio  may  in  turn  have  an 

effect  on  the  breeding  associations  exhibited  by  EL.  EL.  bairdl . 

Howard  (1949)  has  noted  that  "in  areas  where  the  sex  ratio  was  not 

equal,  as  many  as  three  females  lived  in  the  same  nest  box  with  one 


101 


male,  and  as  many  as  three  males  lived  In  one  nest  box  with  one  female" 
(p.  14). 

As  noted  earlier,  although  fL.  maniculatus  do  not  appear  to 
defend  feeding  areas  per  se,  resident  animals  may  prevent 
over-utilization  of  food  supplies  within  their  home  ranges  by 
aggressively  limiting  the  settlement  of  potential  breeders  in  these 
areas.  The  presence  of  an  adequate  food  supply  within  a  home  range  may 
increase  the  probability  of  winter  survival  for  residents  and  their 
offspring.  The  observations  reviewed  in  the  preceeding  discussions 
indicate  that  the  availability  and  distribution  of  food  items  may  have 
major  consequences  not  only  on  the  distribution  and  survival  of  these 
mice,  but  on  the  social  behavior  and  breeding  relationships  exhibited 
by  them  as  we  I  I . 
Peromyscus  polionotus 

In  light  of  several  aspects  of  the  ecology  of  P_t  pol ionotgs, 
choosing  mates  with  high  aggressive  ability  may  be  adaptive  for  members 
of  this  species.  One  ecological  factor  of  primary  importance  to  this 
species  is  the  availability  of  suitable  nest  sites.  Because  they 
construct  their  own  burrows  (Hayne,  1936;  Smith,  1966;  Smith  &  Criss, 
1967)  Ej.  pol ionotus  are  not  restricted  by  the  availability  of 
naturally  occuring  nests  per  se.  This  species  does,  however,  require  a 
fairly  narrow  range  of  soil  conditions  for  nest  construction,  and  In 
habitat  undisturbed  by  man  is  probably  restricted  to  areas  in  early 
successional  stages  (Golley,  Gentry,  Caldwell,  &  Davenport,  1965),  and 
sandy  beach  areas.  According  to  Smith  (1966)  abundance  of  £j. 
pol ionotus  "is  correlated  with  soil  type,  amount  of  soil  drainage 


102 


(Table  2),  type  and  amount  of  vegetation.  All  of  the  habitats  occupied 
by  this  species  are  characterized  by  sparse  vegetation  and  relatively 
well-drained  or  recently  plowed  soils  ..."  (p.  11).  Well-drained  fine 
sand  soils  were  preferred  for  burrowing,  and  mice  "never  constructed 
burrows  in  hard  soils  where  digging  was  difficult,  nor  in  areas  where 
the  hardpan  was  close  to  the  surface  of  the  ground"  (p.  13).  Few  mice 
were  observed  in  heavily  forested  areas,  or  in  areas  with  dense 
vegetation  (Rand  &  Host,  1942;  Smith,  1966;  Personal  observations), 
and  the  number  of  mice  nesting  in  a  given  area  appeared  to  be 
negatively  correlated  with  the  density  of  vegetation  (Rand  &  Host, 
1942;  Smith,  1966).   Individual  mice  construct  several  burrows  in 
close  proximity  (Rand  &  Host,  1942;  Smith,  1966)  within  a  fairly  well 
defined  home  range  (Blair,  1951;  Davenport,  1964)  and  defend  these 
burrows  against  intruders  (Blair,  1951).   It  is  likely  that  in  addition 
to  defending  burrows  within  their  home  range,  individuals  also 
aggressively  exclude  other  potential  settlers  from  suitable  nest  areas 
near  their  burrows.  This  suggestion  is  supported  by  the  observation 
that  often  the  burrows  within  a  given  area  al I  appeared  to  have  been 
constructed  by  a  single  individual,  or  pair  of  individuals  (Rand  & 
Host,  1942;  Smith,  1966),  and  by  observations  in  the  present  study 
that  animals  attacked  and  chased  one  another  in  all  areas  of  the 
semi  natural  apparatus,  and  not  just  in  the  area  that  actually  contained 
their  burrow.  Although  P*.  pol ionotus  defend  burrows  within  their  home 
range  (Blair,  1951),  they  do  not  appear  to  defend  their  entire  home 
range  (Davenport,  1964).  The  apparent  lack  of  defense  of  home  range  by 


103 


E*.  DQl ionotus  may  be  a  reflection  of  the  fact  that  this  species 
utilizes  a  wide  variety  of  food  items  (Gentry  &  Smith,  1968;  Smith, 
1966)  that  are  probably  not  economically  defendable. 

Although  multiple  burrows  may  simply  serve  a  survival  function  as 
refuges  from  predators,  the  maintenance  of  several  burrows  is  more 
likely  to  act  in  some  manner  to  maximize  reproductive  success  in  E*. 
pol ionotus.  Blair  (1951)  and  Rand  and  Host  (1942)  observed  that  P*. 
pol ionotus  change  nests  frequently;  this  frequent  change  of  nest  site 
could  be  a  strategy  to  reduce  the  level  of  parasitic  Infestation  of 
offspring.  Ej.  pol ionotus  have  also  been  observed  to  utilize 
unoccupied  burrows  as  food  caches  (Blair,  1951;  Rand  &  Host,  1942; 
Smith,  1966).  Some  evidence  exists  that  populations  of  this  species 
may  at  times  be  food  limited  (Smith,  1971;  Smith  &  Blessing,  1969); 
cached  food  might  provide  a  food  reserve  that  would  allow  parents  and 
offspring  to  survive  and/or  reproduce  during  such  periods. 
Alternatively  cached  food  could  contain  nutrients  that  ,  although  not 
critical  for  survival,  might  be  critical  for  reproduction.  Food  caches 
may  therefore  allow  breeding  in  seasons  during  which  it  would  not 
otherwise  be  adaptive.  Evidence  supportive  of  these  hypotheses  are 
Smith's  (1966)  observations  that  food-deprived  individuals  of  this 
species  become  torpid  at  very  cool  temperatures  while  non-food-deprived 
individuals  do  not,  and  observations  of  improved  reproductive 
performance  in  pairs  fed  acorns  (a  frequently  cached  food  item) 
parasitized  by  beetle  larvae.  Abundant  cached  food  may  allow 
individuals  to  maintain  a  level  of  activity  compatible  with  breeding, 
even  in  cooler  temperatures,  and  high-quality  food  caches  (e.  g. 


104 


acorns  containing  beetle  larvae)  may  provide  nutrients  that  would 
otherwise  not  be  available  in  winter  months. 

An  additional  benefit  that  may  derive  from  the  maintenance  of 
multiple  burrows  has  been  discussed  by  Foltz  (1979).  Upon  the  birth  of 
a  new  litter  older  offspring  may  move  into  a  nearby  burrcw  with  their 
father.  The  behavior  of  moving  to  nearby  burrows  may  allow  offspring 
to  extend  the  period  during  which  they  may  take  advantage  of  parental 
resources  and  care.  Offspring  that  receive  extended  care  may  be  better 
able  to  compete  for  food  and  to  establish  burrows  when  they  disperse 
than  offspring  that  do  not  receive  such  care.  This  may  be  of  particular 
importance  in  £*  pol ionotus  because  dispersing  individuals  appear  to 
suffer  very  high  mortality  (Smith,  1966,  1968). 

The  maintenance  of  multiple  burrows  by  E*  pol ionotus  appears,  as 
a  result  of  the  factors  discussed,  to  be  a  highly  adaptive  strategy  for 
members  of  this  species.  The  facts  that  E*  pol ionotus  are  restricted 
to  very  specific  types  of  habitat,  and  that  individual  mice  maintain 
several  burrows  in  an  area,  probably  act  as  major  factors  that  limit 
the  size  of  the  breeding  populations  in  this  species.   If  nest  sites 
are  a  limited  and  critical  resource  for  Ej.  pol ionotus.  one  would 
expect  individuals  of  this  species  to  compete  for  them,  and  to  defend 
them  from  conspeci f ics. 

According  to  consensus  polygyny  should  be  generally  be 
advantageous  to  males  (Brown  J.  L.,  1975;  Daly  &  Wilson,  1978; 
Dawkins,  1976;  Orians,  1969;  Verner,  1964),  and  males  should 
therefore  attempt  to  control  limited  resources  In  a  manner  that  allows 
them  to  acquire  additional  mates.  Because  nest  sites  appear  to  be  a 


105 


limited  and  defendable  resource  for  R*.  pol ionotus.  males  of  this 
species  might  be  capable  of  gaining  multiple  mates  by  controlling  areas 
suitable  for  nesting  (see  Verner  &  Willson,  1966,  p.  145).  This 
species  has,  however,  consistently  been  found  to  exhibit  a  monogamous 
mating  system  (Blair,  1951;  Foltz,  1979,  1981;  Rand  &  Host,  1942; 
Smith,  1966). 

According  to  Em  I  en  and  Oring  (1977)  the  two  preconditions  for 
a  species  to  exhibit  a  polygamous  mating  system  are  that  (1) 
individuals  must  be  potentially  capable  of  economically  defending 
multiple  mates,  or  resources  critical  to  gaining  multiple  mates,  and 
(2)  individuals  must  be  able  to  utilize  that  potential.   Several 
factors  may  prevent  R«.  pol ionotus  males  from  fulfilling  these  two 
preconditions  for  polygamy.  Among  the  more  important  of  these  factors 
are  the  distribution  of  nest  sites  and  food  items,  and  it  is  likely 
that  monogamy  in  £*  pol ionotus  rests  upon  considerations  related  to 
both  of  these  factors. 

Avai labi I ity  of  nests.  As  discussed  previously  the  number  of 
suitable  nest  sites  for  fL.  pol ionotus  in  any  given  area  may  be  very 
limited.   It  is  possible  that  nest  sites  for  this  species  may  be  so 
limited  as  to  impose  monogamy  because  two  adults  are  required  for  their 
defense  (See  Wilson,  1975,  p.  330).  Alternatively  a  male  may  be 
capable  of  defending  a  sufficient  number  of  burrows  to  maintain  a 
single  female  and  her  litter,  but  nest  sites  may  be  too  dispersed  for  a 
male  to  defend  a  sufficient  number  of  burrows  to  maintain  two  females. 
Although  the  maintenance  of  multiple  burrows  appears  to  be  important  in 
terms  of  the  ecology  of  JL.  pol ionotus.  in  light  of  the  available  data 


106 


it  is  difficult  to  evaluate  how  many  burrows  a  pair  might  actually 
require  to  maximize  its  reproductive  success.  While  the  availability 
or  distribution  of  nest  sites  alone  might  explain  monogamy  in  Pj. 
pol ionotus,  it  is  most  likely  to  be  of  importance  in  conjunction  with 
other  factors.  One  might  expect  for  example  that  if  food  items  were 
abundant  year-round,  burrows  would  not  be  used  as  food  caches,  and 
males  might  be  more  capable  of  acquiring  additional  females. 

Availability  of  food  items.   If  suitable  habitat  for  burrowing 
was  abundant  a  male  might  be  capable  of  defending  enough  burrows  to 
maintain  several  females.  However,  even  if  burrows  were  abundant, 
females  themselves  may  be  too  widely  dispersed  to  defend  if  food  items 
are  sparse  (CI utton-Brock  &  Harvey,  1977,  1978;  Eisenberg,  1977),  or 
if  food  items  are  so  widely  dispersed  around  nest  areas  as  to  make  it 
highly  unlikely  that  a  female  could  raise  a  litter  without  assistance. 
Under  these  conditions  females  may  require  males  to  display  some 
evidence  of  commitment  (Maynard  Smith,  1977)  prior  to  mating  with  them, 
or  may  impose  monogamy  on  males  by  aggressively  preventing  other 
females  access  to  their  mate  (Wittenberger,  1979,  1981;  Wittenberger  & 
Til  son,  1980). 

Aggression  and  Mate  Selection 
Intraspecif ic  aggression 

Peromyscus  maniculatus.  As  discussed  in  preceeding  sections 
aggression  is  an  important  component  of  the  social  behavior  of 
Peromyscus  maniculatus.  and  an  individual's  aggressive  ability  probably 
plays  a  major  role  in  determining  if  that  individual  will  be  capable  of 
establishing  and  maintaining  itself  as  a  breeding  member  of  the 


107 


population.  In  laboratory  studies  dominant  males  of  this  species  have 
been  found  to  be  more  successful  than  subordinate  males  in  nesting  with 
females  (Blair  &  Howard,  1944;  Eisenberg,  1962),  copulating  with 
females  (Dewsbury,  1979;  1981c),  and  siring  offspring  (Blair  &  Howard, 
1944;  Dewsbury,  1981c).   Dewsbury  (1981c)  has  also  observed  that 
females  tend  to  approach  and  solicit  dominant  males  more  frequently 
than  subordinate  males.  Although  female  approaches  were  confounded 
with  copulatory  behavior  and  male  availability  because  females  "tended 
to  approach  and  solicit  males  with  which  they  were  copulating  and  which 
were  nearby"  and  "this  was  generally  the  dominant  male"  (Dewsbury, 
1981,  p.  892);  these  observations  may  indicate  female  preference  for 
dominant  males  in  this  species.  The  observation  in  the  present  study, 
of  a  preference  by  males  and  females  for  the  more  assertive  of  two 
opposite-sexed  individuals,  is  consistent  with  the  hypothesis  that 
individuals  of  this  species  may  prefer  aggressive  individuals  as  mates. 

Peromyscus  pol ionotus.  Although  some  investigators  (e.g.  Blair  & 
Howard,  1944)  have  observed  only  very  low  levels  of  aggression  in  P*. 
pol ionotuSp  other  investigators  (Garten,  1976;  Smith,  Garten  &  Ramsey, 
1975)  have  observed  higher  levels;  and  these  investigators  have 
proposed  that  aggression  in  Pj.  pol ionotus  is  genetically  based,  and 
that  it  may  play  an  important  role  in  both  the  social  behavior  and 
social  organization  of  this  species.   Information  presented  in  the 
preceeding  discussions  suggested  that  aggressive  ability  may  serve 
important  functions  in  i ntraspecif ic  competition  in  P.  pol ionotus.  and 
may  therefore  be  an  important  factor  in  mate  selection  in  this  species. 
The  results  of  the  present  study  support  the  hypotheses  that  aggressive 


108 


ability  may  be  important  in  the  social  behavior  and  mate  preferences 
exhibited  by  this  species.   In  the  seminatural  experiments  Individuals 
of  both  sexes  displayed  aggressive  behavior,  the  majority  of  which  was 
directed  against  same-sexed  individuals.  This  is  consistent  with  the 
type  of  behavior  Kleiman  (1977)  has  proposed  as  indicative  of  monogamy. 
In  addition  members  of  both  sexes  nested  more  frequently  with  the  more 
aggressive  of  two  opposite-sexed  individuals,  and  members  of  both  sexes 
displayed  significant  preference  for  more  assertive  individuals  of  the 
opposite  sex  in  preference  tests. 
Interspecific  aggression 

Because  individuals  of  most  species  appear  to  compete  with  members 
of  other  species  for  at  least  some  resources,  aggressive  ability  could 
function  in  interspecific  as  well  as  intraspeci f ic  interactions. 
Peromyscus  maniculatus  appears  to  compete  with  other  species  In  many 
parts  of  its  range  (Drickamer,  1978;  Holbrook,  1978;  Kritzman,  1974; 
Redfield,  Krebs,  &  Taitt,  1977),  and  P.  pol ionotus  often  appears  to 
compete  with  Mus  musculus  for  food  (Briese  &  Smith,  1973;  Caldwell  & 
Gentry,  1966a).  Aggressive  competition  for  resources  is  also  common  in 
Microtus;  for  example:  M*.  agrestes  appears  to  compete  with  Mj. 
arval is  for  resources  (Dienske,  1979),  while  both  of  these  species  may 
at  times  be  in  competition  with  Clethr ionomys  glared  us  (DeJonge, 
1979);  Mj.  longicaudus  appears  to  be  excluded  from  its  preferred 
habitat  by  M.  montanus  in  some  localities  (Randall,  1978);  and  Nk 
pennsy I  van  icus  and  M.  ochrogaster  may  also  compete  In  some  areas 
(Miller,  1969).   Individuals  in  most  rodent  populations  are  probably 
exposed  to  at  least  a  moderate  level  of  interspecific  competition  for 


109 


resources,  and  individuals  with  high  aggressive  ability  would  be 
expected  to  fare  better  in  this  competition  than  individuals  of  low 
aggressive  ability,  and  thus  to  be  preferred  as  mates.   Interspecific 
competition  could,  therefore,  function  to  maintain  choice  for 
aggressive  mates  in  many  species — including  those  that  may  not 
exhibit  high  levels  of  i ntraspeci f ic  aggression. 
Farlv  breeding 

Earlier  breeding  (Darwin,  1874)  is  probably  not  a  consideration  in 
the  preference  displayed  for  aggressive  mates  by  Pj_  po| jonotus,  but 
may  be  of  some  importance  to  IL.  maniculatus.   It  is  unlikely  that 
early  breeding  is  an  important  factor  to  P_«.  pol ionotus.  Although 
Ej.  pol ionotus  display  breeding  peaks  and  declines  they  do  not  appear 
to  be  seasonal  breeders  (Davenport,  1964;  Smith,  1966).  Therefore, 
although  some  seasons  may  be  better  for  breeding  than  others  (Smith  & 
McGinnis,  1968),  an  individual  can  not  really  get  an  "earlier  start"  in 
breeding  than  others  in  the  population. 

Peromyscus  maniculatus  on  the  other  hand  do  appear  to  be  seasonal 
breeders,  and  also  form  large  winter  aggregations  that  may  contain 
unrelated  adults  (Howard,  1949).  More-aggressive  individuals  might  be 
capable  of  initiating  breeding  earlier  than  less-aggressive  individuals 
after  dispersal  from  winter  aggregations  when  competition  for  resources 
is  likely  to  be  intense  (Healey,  1967;  Taitt,  1981). 
Bruce  effect 

Exposure  of  females  to  unfamiliar  males  may  result  in  pregnancy 
blockage  (Bruce,  1959;  1960).  This  phenomenon  is  known  as  the  "Bruce 


110 


effect",  and  several  explanations  have  been  offered  for  it.  Many  of 
these  hypotheses  have  been  reviewed  by  Schwagmeyer  (1979),  who  has 
proposed  that  "females  are,  in  effect,  selecting  one  mate  In  preference 
to  another  when  pregnancy  blockage  occurs.  One  would  therefore  predict 
that  the  Bruce  effect  would  be  limited  to  circumstances  in  which  the 
benefits  from  mating  with  the  new  male  outweigh  any  cost  of  the  delay 
in  parturition  or  physiological  effects  involved"  (p.  934). 

This  argument  has  recently  been  extended  by  Huck,  Soltis,  and 
Coopersmith  (1982).  These  investigators  observed  that  dominant  male 
house  mice  (Mus  musculus)  significantly  reduced  the  survival  of  strange 
pups,  whereas  subordinate  males  did  not,  and  that  dominant  males  would 
copulate  with  a  female  after  killing  her  litter.  Previous 
investigators  (Hrdy,  1979;  Labov,  1980;  Mai  lory  &  Brooks,  1980)  had 
suggested  that,  in  species  in  which  males  committed  Infanticide,  it 
would  be  less  costly  for  females  to  block  pregnancy  and  mate  with  a 
strange  male  than  to  lose  the  litter  later  through  infanticide. 
Following  these  suggestions,  their  own  observations,  and  the 
observation  by  Huck  (1982)  that  dominant  males  are  more  effective  in 
initiating  pregnancy  blockage;  Huck  et  a  I . ( 1 982 )  proposed  that  it  is 
most  advantageous  for  females  to  display  pregnancy  blocks  when 
confronted  with  dominant  rather  than  subordinate  males  because  they 
face  the  greatest  risk  of  infanticide  by  these  males.  As  stated  by 
Schwagmeyer  (1979),  by  displaying  pregnancy  blocks  females  are  in 
effect  displaying  a  preference  to  mate  with  aggressive  males;  and  the 
Bruce  effect  would  be  adaptive  in  this  context  as  long  as  the  costs  of 
postponing  the  current  litter  were  outweighed  by  the  benefits  derived 


Ill 


from  mating  with  a  male  of  high  aggressive  ability.  The  Bruce  effect, 
like  the  inciting  behavior  of  female  elephant  seals  (Cox,  1981;  Cox  & 
LeBeouf,  1977),  may  be  a  means  by  which  a  female  ensures  that  she  mates 
with  males  with  high  aggressive  ability. 
Heritable  aggressive  ability 

The  benefits  of  selecting  mates  with  high  aggressive  ability  that 
have  been  discussed  to  this  point  are  not  dependent  upon  any  component 
of  an  animal's  aggressive  behavior  being  heritable.  The  expression  of 
many  of  the  components  of  aggressive  behavior  has,  however,  been 
demonstrated  to  be  at  least  partial ly  under  genetic  control  (Scott, 
1966;  Scott  &  Frederickson,  1951;  Simon,  1979).  The  components  of 
aggression  may  be  divided  into  two  major  categories:  components 
related  to  the  tendency  for  an  animal  to  display  aggression,  or  its 
"aggressiveness"  ,  and  components  related  to  the  ability  of  an  animal 
to  effectively  perform  aggressive  behaviors.  The  validity  of  this 
division  is  supported  by  research  reviewed  and  discussed  by  Scott 
(1966),  and  by  Scott  and  Frederickson  (1951).  As  stated  by  Scott 
(1966)  "Heredity  produces  important  differences  in  fighting  behavior 
between  mouse  strains,  some  being  more  easily  excited  to  fight  than 
others  and  some  strains  being  more  capable  of  winning  than  others"  (p. 
691). 

Although  an  animal's  tendency  toward  aggression  and  its  ability  to 
perform  aggressive  behavior  effectively  may  be  separable,  they  would  be 
expected  to  be  highly  positively  correlated.  One  would  expect  that  it 
would  not  be  adaptive  for  an  animal  to  be  highly  prone  to  engage  In 
aggressive  encounters  If  it  stood  little  chance  of  winning  those 


112 


encounters;  nor  on  the  other  hand  would  it  be  expected  to  be  adaptive 
for  an  animal  to  be  completely  unprovokable  if  it  had  high  aggressive 
ability.  Because  fights  involve  potential  costs  as  well  as  potential 
benefits  (Maynard  Smith,  1976;  Maynard  Smith  &  Price,  1973), 
individuals  should  assess  opponents  carefully,  and  fight  only  when  they 
have  a  reasonable  expectation  of  winning  (CI utton-Brock  &  Albon,  1979; 
Clutton-Brock,  Albon,  Gibson  &  Guinness,  1979).   Individuals  that  most 
frequently  display  aggressive  behavior,  therefore,  should  normally  also 
be  those  individuals  that  have  the  highest  aggressive  ability.   If  the 
display  of  aggression  is  an  indication  of  an  individual's  aggressive 
ability,  then  these  displays  may  be  used  to  evaluate  that  individual's 
potential  as  a  mate  (Cox,  1981;  Cox  &  LeBeouf,  1977).  While,  as 
previously  noted,  many  of  the  benefits  that  may  accrue  to  individuals 
that  chose  mates  with  high  aggressive  ability  do  not  depend  on  that 
ability  being  heritable,  choosing  aggressive  mates  should  be  even  more 
adaptive  if  any  of  the  components  of  aggressive  ability  are  heritable. 
Individuals  selecting  aggressive  mates  would  gain  not  only  immediate 
benefits,  such  as  better  territory  or  defense  of  nest  site,  but  "good 
genes"  (Maynard  Smith,  1956;  Trivers,  1972)  as  well.   (However  see, 
Krebs  &  Davies,  1981;  Maynard  Smith,  1978,  p.  170,  171;  Parker, 
1979,  p.  146;  for  limits  on  the  use  of  heritable  factors  as  a  basis 
for  choice).  Selection  of  mates  with  high  aggressive  ability  may  be 
adaptive  even  when  such  individuals  do  not  hold  the  best  available 
resources,  if  these  individuals  are  more  attractive  as  mates,  and 
aggressive  ability  is  heritable.  This  combination  of  conditions  has 


113 


been  suggested  to  act  to  lower  the  polygny  threshold  in  species  (the 
"sexy  son"  hypothesis:  Heisler,  1981;  Weatherhead  &  Robertson,  1979). 

Some  observations  of  Peromyscus  maniculatus  bairdi  and  P. 
pol ionotus  suggest  that  the  display  of  aggression,  or  displays  related 
to  aggressive  ability,  may  provide  a  basis  for  mate  selection  in  these 
species.  Dewsbury's  (1981b)  observation  that  FL.  maniculatus  females 
approach  and  solicit  dominant  males  more  frequently  than  subordinates 
in  two-male  copu I atory  tests,  although  open  to  other  intrepretations, 
is  also  consistent  with  this  suggestion.   In  the  present  study 
high-aggression  JL.  pol ionotus  males  displayed  aggressive  digging  more 
frequently  than  low-aggression  males,  and  may  act  to  prevent  the 
display  of  this  behavior  by  low-aggression  males.  The  areas  around 
nest  sites  of  this  species  often  exhibit  evidence  of  frequent  digging 
behavior.  Although  members  of  this  species  do  not  appear  to  exhibit 
territorial  behavior  (Davenport,  1964)  they  do  defend  nest  burrows 
(Blair,  1951),  and  most  likely  also  some  of  the  area  around  burrows. 
The  "incipient  burrows"  and  other  evidence  of  digging  near  nest  burrows 
may  provide  an  indication  to  individuals  that  an  area  Is  occupied;  and 
aggressive  digging  may  be  a  means  of  displaying  an  ability  to  construct 
and  defend  burrows. 

Familiarity  as  a  Factor  in  Preference 
The  results  of  the  present  study  were  not  consistent  with  the 
hypothesis  that  members  of  monogamous  species  should  display  greater 
preference  for  familiar  than  unfamiliar  individuals,  nor  with  the 
hypothesis  that  the  effects  of  familiarity  on  the  choice  of  members  of 
polygamous  species  should  be  negligible.   In  familiarity  preference 


114 


tests  individuals  of  both  sexes  of  the  polygamous  species  Peromyscus 
maniculatus  displayed  preference  for  familiar  individuals  of  the 
opposite  sex,  while  individuals  of  the  monogamous  species  Peromyscus 
pol ionotus  did  not  display  preference.   In  addition,  Et  pol ionotus  did 
not  display  preference  for  nesting  with  familiar  individuals  of  the 
opposite  sex  in  the  semi  natural  experiments.  Two  types  of  factors  may 
have  influenced  the  behavior  of  _P.  maniculatus  and  £*.  pol Ionotus  in 
familiarity  preference  tests;  these  are  1)  factors  related  to  the 
prior  history  of  the  animals  tested  and  2)  factors  related  to  the 
ecology  and  social  system  of  these  particular  species. 
Prior  History 

The  ability  to  recognize  differences  in  the  familiarity  of 
individual  odors  has  been  demonstrated  in  a  wide  variety  of  mammalian 
(Brown,  R.E.,  1979;  Halpin,  1980)  and  non-mammalian  (Hal pf n,  1980) 
species;  and  evidence  exists  that  individuals  of  many  rodent  species 
may  exhibit  preferences  based  on  this  ability  (Brown,  R.E.,  1979).  Of 
particular  relevance  to  the  present  discussion  are  observations 
suggesting  that  an  individual's  preference  for  familiar  or  unfamiliar 
conspecifics  may  be  determined  in  part  by  whether  that  individual 
previously  received  monogamous  or  polygamous  mating  experience.  While 
monogamously  mated  female  rats  appear  to  prefer  the  odor  of  a  familiar 
over  a  novel  male,  polygamous  I y  mated  females  display  no  preference, 
polygamously  mated  males  prefer  novel  females,  and  monogamously  mated 
males  may  either  display  no  preference  or  preference  for  novel  females 
depending  on  test  conditions  (Carr,  Krames,  &  Costanzo,  1970;  Carr  et 
al.,  1979;  Carr  et  al.,  1980;  Krames  et  al.,  1967).  The  general 


115 


tendency  in  these  studies  appears  to  be  greater  preference  for  familiar 
individuals  after  monogamous  mating  experience  and  greater  preference 
for  novel  individuals  after  polygamous  mating  experience.  Dewsbury 
(1979)  has  also  observed  a  higher  probability  of  mating  In  fL.  nu 
bairdi  in  tests  in  which  individuals  were  familiar  (had  previous 
monogamous  mating  experience)  than  in  tests  in  which  mates  were 
unf ami  I iar. 

Prior  to  familiarity  preference  tests  animals  in  the  present  study 
were  housed  with  a  single  opposite-sexed  conspecific  and  therefore, 
outside  the  possibility  of  having  mated  with  siblings,  would  have  had 
only  monogamous  mating  opportunities  prior  to  these  tests.  While 
pretest  housing  conditions  may  in  part  explain  the  preference  for 
familiar  individuals  displayed  by  JL.  maniculatus.  they  can  not  explain 
the  lack  of  preference  displayed  by  fL.  pol  ionotus.  The  lack  of 
preference  displayed  by  fL.  pol ionotus  in  the  familiarity  preference 
tests,  although  consistent  with  observations  of  the  nesting  behavior  of 
this  species  in  the  seminatural  study,  is  inconsistent  with  predictions 
based  on  the  mating  system  of  this  species.  These  differences  in  the 
preference  of  P.  maniculatus  and  P_».  pol  ionotus  may  be  explained  by 
differences  in  the  ecology  and  social  organization  of  these  two 
species. 

Ecology  and  Social  System 
Peromyscus  maniculatus 

As  noted  previously  breeding  fL.  eu.  bairdi  appear  to  maintain 
overlapping  home  ranges  (Blair,  1940).  Although  breeding  adults  appear 
to  aggressively  limit  settlement  of  strange  juveniles  in  their  home 


116 


range  (Ayer  &  Whittsett,  1980;  Enders,  1978,  Whitsett  et  al.,  1979) 
little  overt  aggression  appears  to  occur  between  members  of  established 
populations  (Hill,  1977;  Terman,  1961,  1974).  Lack  of  aggression 
between  established  residents  In  a  population  is  not  uncommon,  and  may 
be  a  result  of  the  "dear  enemy"  phenomenon  as  described  by  Wilson 
(1975):  "A  territorial  neighbor  is  not  ordinarily  a  threat.   It  should 
pay  to  recognize  him  as  an  individual,  to  agree  mutually  upon  the  joint 
boundary,  and  to  waste  as  little  energy  as  possible  in  hostile 
exchanges  thereafter"  (p.  273).  This  effect  appears  to  occur  in 
at  least  some  (Healey,  1967),  but  not  all  (Vestal  &  He  I  lack,  1978) 
subspecies  of  £*.  manicu  latus. 

The  dear  enemy  phenomenon  may  be  a  factor  in  juvenile  dispersal 
and  settlement  in  Eju  maniculatus.  Blair  (1958)  observed  that  the 
majority  of  juveniles  in  a  population  of  E_l   maniculatus  in  Texas 
dispersed  either  very  short  distances  from  the  natal  site  or  not  at 
all;  and  38  percent  of  the  females  and  28  percent  of  the  males  in  a 
population  of  R*.  el.  bairdi  in  Michigan  did  not  disperse  from  their 
natal  home  range  to  breed  (Dice  &  Howard,  1951).  Healey  (1967)  has 
suggested  that  for  £,.  maniculatus  "an  animal's  chances  of  breeding  are 
severely  limited  when  it  moves  any  distance  from  its  birth  place"  (p. 
388).  These  observations  suggest  that  a  juvenile  has  the  greatest 
probability  of  breeding  successfully  if  it  remains  on  or  near  the 
parental  home  range.   In  light  of  the  aggression  directed  toward 
unfamiliar  juveniles  by  adults  (Ayer  &  Whitsett,  1980;  Enders,  1978; 
Healey,  1967;  Sadleir,  1965)  one  of  the  more  successful  strategies  for 


117 


juvenile  £».  man  leu latus  may  be  to  breed  with  its  neighbors  rather  than 
to  attempt  to  disperse  and  breed  with  unfamiliar  conspecif ics. 

Early  breeding  may  be  an  additional  factor  acting  to  increase  the 
adaptive  value  of  selecting  familiar  mates  in  this  species.  As  Is 
typical  of  other  Peromyscus  (Terman,  1968),  E*.  maniculatus  tends  to 
remain  in  a  particular  area  once  it  has  established  itself  as  a 
breeding  resident  of  that  area.  The  apparent  sedentary  nature  of 
residents,  and  the  overlapping  home  ranges  they  exhibit  (Blair,  1940; 
Howard,  1949;  Mihok,  1979;  Morris,  1955),  probably  result  in  each 
resident  breeding  within  a  fairly  well  defined  group  of  familiar 
conspecif ics.   Individuals  within  an  area  aggregate  in  the  winter  and 
disperse  from  these  aggregations  to  breed  in  the  spring  (Howard,  1949; 
Metzgar,  1979).   Individuals  that  have  become  familiar  In  overwintering 
aggregations  or  through  prior  breeding  may  be  able  to  pair  more 
quickly,  and  establish  themselves  as  breeders  earlier  in  the  spring 
than  unfamiliar  Individuals. 
Peromyscus  pol ionotus 

Peromyscus  pol ionotus  are  sedentary  once  they  become  resident  in 
an  area  (Blair,  1951;  Smith,  1966,  1968),  are  not  seasonal  breeders 
(Davenport,  1964;  Smith,  1966;  Smith  &  McGinnis,  1968),  and  exhibit  a 
monogamous  mating  system  in  which  they  form  long-term  reproductive 
pairs  (Blair,  1951;  Foltz,  1979,  1981).   In  addition,  members  of  this 
species  are  generally  fairly  short  lived  (Dapson,  1972).  As  a  result 
of  these  factors  the  opportunities  for  members  of  this  species  to 
utilize  familiarity  as  a  factor  in  mate  selection  during  its  lifetime 
may  be  somewhat  limited.  Two  situations  that  could  occur  in  which 


118 


familiarity  may  be  a  factor  In  mate  selection  in  this  species  would  be 
in  the  initial  selection  of  a  mate  upon  attaining  reproductive 
maturity,  or  in  the  selection  of  a  new  mate  upon  the  death  or 
incapacitation  of  the  current  mate.  These  possibilities  will  be 
examined  in  the  following  discussion. 

As  in  L  maniculatus.  L*.  pol  ionotus  exhibit  overlapping  home 
ranges  that  do  not  appear  to  be  defended  against  breeding  adult 
conspecifics  (Davenport,  1965).   It  Is  likely  that  the  majority  of 
aggressive  interactions  in  IL.  pol ionotus.  other  than  possibly 
aggression  incidental  to  foraging,  are  centered  around  the  defense  of 
nest  sites  (Blair,  1951).   In  light  of  the  specific  nesting 
requirements  of  this  species,  nest  site  defense  would  probably 
effectively  limit  juvenile  recruitment  into  the  resident  breeding 
population  since  it  is  unlikely  that  a  juvenile  could  obtain  nest  sites 
in  competition  with  adult  conspecifics.  A  juvenile  could  breed  near 
the  natal  home  range  if  it  were  able  to  attract  a  neighboring  resident 
animal  as  a  mate.  Opportunities  for  a  juvenile  to  enter  a  breeding 
relationship  with  a  familiar  resident  are,  however,  likely  to  be  very 
limited.  Established  residents  may  be  exposed  to  the  choice  of 
juvenile  mates  only  in  the  event  of  the  reproductive  failure  or  death 
of  their  existing  mates.   Juveniles,  however,  would  generally  be 
expected  to  perform  more  poorly  in  competition  with  residents  than 
would  an  adult  mate.   In  addition  a  juvenile's  reproductive  performance 
is  unproven,  and  at  least  for  females  generally  below  that  of  adults 
(Caldwell  &  Gentry,  1965b;  Smith,  1966;  Williams,  Gol ley,  &  Carmon, 
1965).  The  best  choice  for  a  resident  adult  after  the  loss  of  a  mate, 


119 


therefore,  would  probably  be  to  mate  with  a  neighboring  resident 
rather  than  a  juvenile;  or  barring  the  availability  of  a  neighboring 
resident  to  mate  with  a  transient  adult. 

Although  small  groups  of  young  sexually  immature  R*.  pol ionotus 
have  been  observed  in  the  winter  (Smith,  1966),  it  is  unlikely  that 
familiarity  from  relationships  in  these  groups  has  a  general  effect  on 
mate  selection  in  this  species.  These  groups  comprised  less  than  three 
percent  of  the  social  groups  observed  by  Smith  (1966),  and  such  groups 
were  never  observed  by  Rand  and  Host  (1942).  Smith  (1968)  has 
suggested  that  these  groups  are  composed  of  I  itter  mates  that  are 
overwintering  in  parental  burrows.  Several  observations  lend  support 
to  this  suggestion.  First,  adult  defense  of  burrows  (Blair,  1951) 
makes  it  unlikely  that  unrelated  individuals  would  be  tolerated  in 
burrows.   In  addition,  evidence  reviewed  by  Foltz  (1979)  suggests  that 
parental  males  may  move  with  litters  to  burrows  near  the  natal  burrow. 
Finally,  the  largest  of  these  groups  observed  by  Smith  (1966)  was 
composed  of  six  individuals,  which  is  within  the  range  of  litter  sizes 
reported  for  P.  pol ionotus  (Laffoday,  1957;  Smith,  1966;  Williams, 
Gol ley  &  Carmon,  1965) . 

Kinship  as  a   Factor  in  Preference 

Although  inbreeding  may  be  advantageous  under  certain  conditions 
(Bengtsson,  1978;  Cowan,  1979;  Maynard  Smith,  1978),  the  generally 
detrimental  effects  of  inbreeding,  such  as  inbreeding  depression,  have 
likely  led  to  the  evolution  of  mechanisms  to  avoid  inbreeding  in  most 
species  (Bixler,  1981).  Evidence  available  for  the  two  species  of 
interest  in  the  present  study,  P_,_  maniculatus  and  P_l  pol  ionotus, 


120 


suggests  both  high  and  low  levels  of  Inbreeding  for  these  species.   In 

the  following  discussion  evidence  for  and  against  inbreeding  In  these 

two  species,  and  the  relevant  observations  from  the  present  study,  will 

be  discussed  in  the  context  of  the  ecology  and  social  behavior  of  these 

species. 

Inbreeding  in  Peromyscus  Maniculatus 

Evidence  for  and  against  inbreeding 

Howard  (1949)  noted  that  in  the  population  of  P.^.  nu.  bairdi  he 
he  studied  "There  seemed  to  be  no  bar  to  the  mating  of  close  relatives, 
and  parent-offspring  matings  and  pairing  between  sibs  occurred  when 
conditions  were  such  that  these  related  mice  happened  to  be  together  at 
the  time  when  they  became  sexually  active"  (p.  15).  Howard  (1949) 
estimated  that  up  to  10  percent  of  the  matings  in  the  population  he 
observed  were  inbred.  Rasmussen  (1964)  has  also  suggested  a  high  level 
of  inbreeding  for  £*.  nu.  graci  I  is  based  on  the  observation  of  a 
shortage  of  heterozygotes  in  the  population  he  observed.  Foltz  (1979, 
1981b)  has,  however,  criticized  both  the  Howard  (1949)  and  Rasmussen 
(1964)  studies  on  the  basis  of  methodological  flaws.  This  criticism 
seems  well  founded  on  the  basis  of  evidence  cited  by  Foltz  (1979),  and 
by  observations  that  reproductive  performance  in  sibling  matings  in 
this  species  is  below  that  for  nonsibling  matings  (Dewsbury,  1982; 
Hill,  1974).  Although  differences  in  the  preference  of  P.  maniculatus 
for  siblings  and  nonsiblings  in  the  present  study  were  statistically 
non-significant,  scores  in  the  majority  of  comparisons  were  higher  for 
nonsibl  ings  than  for  sibl  ings;  and  P_*.  maniculatus  females  that  were 
not  in  diestrus  displayed  significantly  greater  preference  for 


121 


nonsibling  males  than  did  females  in  diestrus.  These  observations  are 
consistent  with  the  hypothesis  that  E*.  maniculatus  should  prefer 
nonsib lings  as  mates  over  siblings. 
Ecological  and  social  factors 

Because  JL.  DU.  bairdi  may  exhibit  inbreeding  depression  (Dewsbury, 
1982a;  HIM,  1974)   individuals  of  this  species  would  be  expected  to 
display  stronger  preference  for  nonsib lings  than  was  exhibited  in  the 
present  study.  The  small  differences  in  preference  exhibited  in  the 
present  study  might  be  explained  i  f  P*.  m*.  bairdi.  although  attracted 
to  nonsiblings  as  mates,  are  also  attracted  to  siblings  on  other  bases. 

It  might  be  adaptive  for  siblings  to  be  attracted  to  one  another 
if  by  remaining  together  they  were  able  to  increase  direct  benefits  to 
themselves  (e.  g.,  through  increased  survival),  or  if  such  behavior 
resulted  in  an  increase  in  their  inclusive  fitness  (Hamilton,  W.  D., 
1964,  a,  b).  At  least  two  factors  in  the  behavior  of  L.  Dk  bairdi 
make  it  probable  that  they  will  maintain  extensive  contact  with 
relatives,  including  siblings,  and  thereby  provide  opportunities  for 
kin  selected  behavior  in  this  species.  First,  P*.  nu  bairdi  tend  to 
disperse  short  distances  from  their  birthplace  before  establishing  a 
home  range  (Dice  &  Howard,  1951;  Howard,  1949).   It  is  likely 
therefore  that  many  of  the  residents  in  a  population  will  have  settled 
near  relatives.   In  addition,  because  winter  aggregations  may  Include 
nearby  residents  (Howard,  1949),  it  is  likely  that  at  least  some  of 
these  relatives  are  likely  to  overwinter  together. 

Participation  in  winter  aggregations  may  greatly  increase  an 
individual's  probability  of  surviving  until  the  spring  breeding  season 


122 


(Howard,  1951).  The  ability  to  remain  in  aggregations  through  the 
winter  appears  to  depend  largely  on  the  availability  of  adequate  food 
to  maintain  the  aggregated  mice  (Howard,  1951).  Although  mice  could 
venture  out  of  aggregations  to  forage,  this  behavior  would  be  somewhat 
self-defeating.  Food  items  will  likely  be  scarce  in  winter  months  and 
difficult  to  find,  and  increased  exposure  to  cold  during  foraging  will 
result  in  increased  heat  loss,  making  it  necessary  to  forage  even  more 
intensely  to  gather  sufficient  food  to  maintain  body  temperature  at  an 
adequate  level.  The  amount  of  food  cached  near  an  aggregation  of  ILl 
DL.  bairdi  may  therefore  determine  how  successful  ly  members  of  that 
aggregation  survive  the  winter. 

Although  information  is  not  available  as  to  what  factors  determine 
where  aggregations  are  formed,  the  nucleus  of  these  aggregations  is 
generally  a  parental  pair  and  one  of  their  litters  (Howard,  1949).   It 
is  likely  that  parents  cache  food  near  the  time  of  the  birth  of  their 
last  fall  litter,  and  that  the  combination  of  this  small  aggregation 
and  food  cache  may  often  attract  additional  neighboring  individuals. 
Because  many  of  these  individuals  may  be  related  to  the  family  group, 
inclusive  fitness  may  be  increased  by  allowing  them  to  join  the 
aggregation  and  utilize  the  food  cache.  Two  additional  factors, 
however,  may  be  of  importance  in  this  context.  First,  at  least  up  to  a 
point,  Increasing  the  size  of  an  aggregation  may  benefit  all  of  its 
members  because  a  larger  group  can  more  easily  maintain  a  higher 
temperature.  This  may  in  part  explain  the  observation  that  individuals 
of  other  species  are  sometimes  allowed  in  aggregations  (Howard,  1949). 
Second,  laboratory  observations  (Rice,  1972;  Terman,  1974)  suggest 


123 


that  an  existing  food  cache  may  act  as  a  stimulus  for  hoarding  behavior 
in  Ea  su   bairdi.   If  individuals  joining  an  aggregation  also  add  to 
the  food  cache  they  may  in  effect  "pay  their  own  way"  as  members  of  the 
aggregation.  The  behaviors  exhibited  by  at  least  some  of  the 
individuals  in  aggregations,  therefore,  may  be  mutual istic. 

Whether  the  behavior  of  an  individual  in  an  aggregation  is  viewed 
as  mutual istic,  or  kin  selected  (or  both),  will  depend  on  the  costs  of 
that  behavior  to  the  individual,  and  upon  who  receives  the  benefits  of 
that  behavior.  The  behavior  of  Individuals  of  other  species  in 
aggregations  of  E*  maniculatus.  for  example,  may  be  mutual  istic.  The 
costs  and  benefits  to  the  resident  parental  pair,  however,  are  much 
more  complex.  Costs  Incurred  by  these  animals  in  caching  food  include 
expenditure  of  time  and  energy,  and  possibly  increased  exposure  to 
predators.  An  additional  possible  cost  of  aggregation  is  related  to 
the  fact  that  abundant  food  In  the  spring  may  a  I  low  pairs  to  breed 
early  (Gashwiler,  1979;  Taitt,  1981).   If  food  caches  are  severely 
depleted  during  winter  aggregation,  spring  breeding  may  be  delayed  for 
the  parental  pair.   In  return  for  these  costs  the  parental  pair  provide 
benefits  to  themselves,  their  offspring,  and  possibly  neighboring 
siblings.  Kin  selection  may  therefore  act  as  one  of  the  factors  that 
maintain  winter  aggregations  in  £».  nu.   bairdi,  and  may  therefore  be 
one  of  the  factors  that  act  to  maintain  attraction  to  siblings  outside 
of  a  breeding  context.  Although  this  hypothesis  is  untested,  the 
relatedness  of  individuals  within  aggregations  could  be  assessed 
through  electrophoretic  and  trap-retrap  studies. 


124 


The  tendency  toward  philopatry  in  P^  maniculatus  may  also  lead 
to  opportunities  to  increase  inclusive  fitness  through  cooperative 
breeding.  Communal  litters,  and  apparently  cooperative  care  of  these 
litters,  have  been  observed  in  populations  of  P*.  maniculatus  in  Texas 
(Blair,  1958),  Colorado  (Hansen,  1957),  and  Michigan  (Pj.  m^  bajrdi; 
Howard,  1949).  Many  of  the  hypotheses  on  the  effects  of  ecological  and 
kinship  factors  on  cooperative  breeding  have  recently  been  reviewed 
(Koenig  &  Pitelka,  1981)  and  will  not  be  discussed  in  detail  here.  The 
generally  accepted  hypothesis  is  "that  habitat  saturation  provides  the 
primary  impetus  for  philopatry,  and  through  it  for  evolution  of  group 
territoriality  and  cooperative  breeding.  .  ."  (Emlen,  1982,  p. 32).  As 
described  by  Emlen  (1982)  "As  population  numbers  increase,  suitable 
habitat  becomes  filled  or  'saturated'.  Unoccupied  terri tori  ties  are 
rare,  and  territory  turnovers  are  few.   As  the  intensity  of  competition 
for  space  increases,  fewer  and  fewer  individuals  are  able  to  establish 
themselves  on  quality  territories.  The  option  of  breeding 
independently  becomes  increasingly  limited"  (p.  32).  An  additional 
factor  that  may  mediate  the  occurrence  of  cooperative  breeding  in  Pt 
maniculatus  is  the  sex  ratic.   Howard  (1949)  has  noted  that  breeding 
combinations  with  more  than  one  individual  of  either  sex  occur  in  areas 
where  the  sex  ratio  is  not  equal.  The  relatedness  of  the  individuals 
in  these  breeding  groups  is  unknown.   A  reasonable  hypothesis,  in  light 
of  the  social  organization  and  behavior  of  this  species,  is  that  under 
conditions  of  high  population  density  and  unequal  sex  ration,  same-sexed 
siblings  of  the  "surplus"  sex  may  find  it  more  adaptive  to  establish 


125 


themselves  in  breeding  groups  with  a  member  of  the  opposite  sex  than  to 
attempt  to  gain  resident  breeding  status  on  their  own. 
Inbreeding  in  Peromyscus  Pol ionotus 
Evidence  for  and  against  inbreeding 

Smith  (1966)  and  Smith,  Carmon,  and  Gentry  (1972)  have  presented 
evidence  that  fLu  pol ionotus  may  be  highly  inbred.  This  finding 
appears  to  be  at  odds  with  evidence  that  reproductive  performance  in  P_«. 
pol ionotus  is  positively  correlated  with  genie  heterozygosity  (Smith  et 
al.,  1975).  Smith  et  al .  (1975)  have  suggested  that  the  level  of 
inbreeding  in  this  species  is  linked  in  an  adaptive  manner  to 
population  density,  level  of  aggression,  and  dispersal.  These 
investigators  suggest  that  at  low  and  increasing  population  densities 
levels  of  aggression  and  dispersion  will  also  be  low,  while  levels  of 
inbreeding  will  be  high.  When  population  density  rises  individuals 
will  begin  to  outbreed  more,  and  produce  more-aggressive  heterozygous 
offspring  that  are  better  suited  to  competition  in  the  population  or 
during  dispersal.  Smith  (1968)  has  also  suggested  that  opposite-sexed 
siblings  display  a  tendency  to  disperse  together,  and  has  observed  that 
the  females  in  these  sibling  pairs  may  often  be  pregnant. 

Foltz  (1979,  1981)  has  suggested  alternative  interpretations  for 
many  of  the  observations  presented  by  these  investigators  as  evidence 
of  inbreeding  in  fL.  pol ionotus.  These  interpretations  are  however 
only  presented  as  alternatives,  and  Foltz  (1979,  1981)  was  not  able  to 
exclude  the  possibility  of  high  levels  of  inbreeding  In  this  species. 
In  light  of  Smith's  (1966)  observation  of  a  preference  by  P_t. 
pol ionotus  females  for  sibling  males  over  nonsibling  males  Foltz  (1981) 


126 


suggested  a  need  for  additional  research  on  the  mating  preferences  of 

P.  pol ionotus.  The  present  study  indicates  that  females  R,. 

pol ionotus.  as  suggested  by  Smith  (1966),  display  a  preference  for 

siblings.  Males  of  this  species  also  tend  to  display  higher  sibling 

than  nonsibling  scores  on  preference  measures,  although  their 

comparisons  were  nonsignificant. 

Ecological  and  social  factors 

The  apparent  tendency  toward  inbreeding  in  Rj.  pol ionotus  (Smith, 
1966,  1968;  Smith,  Carmon  &  Gentry,  1972)  may  be  mediated  by  the  very 
specific  habitat  requirements  of  this  species,  and  the  probability  that 
individuals  must  disperse  long  distances  to  find  new  patches  of 
favorable  environment.  Shields  (1982)  has  suggested  that  "if 
conditions  existed  that  favored  relatively  faithful  transmission  of 
parental  genomes,  then  inbreeding  could  be  favored  over  both  asexual  ity 
and  outbreeding  (p.  264)....  Owing  to  its  flexibility  and  capacity  to 
transmit  successful  parental  genomes  with  maximum  fidelity,  inbreeding 
is  expected  to  be  common  in  organisms  produced  by  stable 
lineage-environment  associations"  (p.  274).  The  very  specific  habitat 
requirements  of  R*.  pol ionotus  may  result  in  such  a  stable 
lineage-environment  association  in  this  species.  Because  habitat 
requirements  for  this  species  are  so  specific  (Rand  &  Host,  1942; 
Smith,  1966,  1968),  offspring  are  likely  to  be  most  successful  If  they 
breed  in  areas  in  which  conditions  vary  little  from  those  of  their 
birth  place. 


127 


The  patch  iness  of  suitable  environment  for  P^.  po|  jonotus  may  also 
predispose  this  species  to  inbreeding  through  selective  pressures 
similar  to  those  that  hae  been  proposed  to  operate  on  Microtus 
pennsylvanicus  (Batzli  et  al.,  1977;  Getz,  1978).   In  reference  to  the 
breeding  habits  of  M.  pennsylvanicus  Batzli  et  al.  (1977)  note  that 
"Microtus  pennsylvanicus  .  .  .  occupies  smaller  patches  of  moist  meadow 
or  marsh.  Under  these  circumstances,  strange  mates  may  not  always  be 
available,  and  it  would  be  disadvantageous  if  siblings  could  not  breed 
with  one  another.  ...   If  ii.  pennsylvanicus  must  continual ly  locate 
and  repopulate  isolated  patches,  the  offspring  of  the  founder(s)  must 
mate  in  order  to  assure  success."  (p.  590) 

Similarly,  for  P^.  pol ionotus.  Smith  (1968)  noted  that 

these  mice  are  found  characteristically  in  habitats  of 
early  stages  of  primary  or  secondary  succession  (Golley 
et  al.,  1965).  For  this  reason  it  is  likely  that  large 
distances  between  suitable  habitat  exist,  and  with  time 
succession  makes  the  habitat  unsuitable  for  the  mice. 
The  pine  forest  habitats  which  they  are  associated  with 
on  the  mainland  are  fire  subclimaxes  (Laessle  1958a, 
1958b;  Smith,  1966)  and  utilization  of  available  habitat 
might  require  certain  individuals  to  disperse  long 
distances  to  find  recently  burned  areas,   (p.  49) 

Smith  (1968),  as  noted  previously  in  this  discussion,  has  also  observed 

a  tendency  for  opposi te-sexed  sibling  pairs  of  FL.  po| ionotus  in 

breeding  condition  to  disperse  together. 

Bateson  (1978,  1979)  has  suggested  that  animals  learn 

characteristics  of  parents  and  siblings,  and  then  use  this  information 

to  choose  individuals  that  are  only  slightly  different  from  kin  as 

mates.  Gilder  and  Slater  (1978)  have  observed  behavior  in  mice  that 

appears  to  conform  to  this  rule,  and  a  similar  rule  of  thumb  may 

provide  a  basis  for  the  apparently  cyclic  preference  for  siblings 


128 


observed  by  Smith  et  al.   (1972).  The  cyclic  changes  in  sibling 
preference  that  may  occur  in  this  species  could  be  generated  by  the 
rule  "choose  siblings  as  mates  if  they  are  not  ±02  similar".  For  El 
pol ionotus  this  may  mean  "prefer  siblings  as  mates  as  long  as  your 
parents  were  not  the  product  of  a  sibling  mating". 

Garten  (1976)  has  observed  a  positive  correlation  between 
aggression  and  genie  heterozygosity  in  E*.  pol ionotus.  and  (Garten, 
1977)  between  genie  heterozygosity  and  exploratory  behavior.  As 
offspring  from  nonsibling  matings  are  more  heterozygous  than  offspring 
from  sibling  matings,  offspring  from  nonsibling  matings  should  be  more 
likely  to  disperse.   In  light  of  the  previous  discussion  on  the 
patchiness  of  the  environment  for  this  species,  it  may  be  adaptive  for 
them  to  disperse  to  new  breeding  habitat  as  sibling  pairs  (as  observed 
by  Smith,  1966)  and  for  offspring  of  these  pairs  to  breed  with 
siblings.  Smith  et  al.   (1975)  have  observed  low  levels  of 
heterozygosity  In  £j.  pol ionotus  populations  at  low  and  early  stages 
of  increase  in  population  density,  which  may  indicate  that  individuals 
at  this  stage  of  population  growth  may  in  fact  be  inbreeding.  The 
offspring  of  the  second  generation  in  the  new  habitat,  however,  being 
the  offspring  of  inbred  parents,  would  be  expected  to  choose 
nonsiblings  as  mates.  As  the  population  density  peaked  many  of  the 
offspring  from  these  outbred  matings  would  be  expected  to  disperse  with 
siblings  and  renew  the  cycle.  At  the  stages  of  late  population  rise 
and  early  decline  then,  the  level  of  heterozygosity  in  the  population 
would  be  expected  to  be  relatively  high,  as  observed  by  Smith  et  al. 
(1975). 


129 


Evolution  of  Monogamy  in  Peromyscus  Pol  ionotus 
Peromyscus  pol lonotus  is  morphologically  more  similar  to  the 
"prairie  forms"  of  JL.  maniculatus  (e.g.  Pj.  ULl  bairdi  or  P.  m. 
pal lescens)  than  to  the  "forest  forms"  of  this  species  (Hooper,  1968), 
and  most  likely  originated  from  one  of  the  prairie  forms  of  P_j. 
maniculatus  during  the  Pleistocene  interglacial  stages  (Blair,  1950). 
As  discussed  previously,  although  Ejl  pol ionotus  and  P.  maniculatus 
are  closely  related,  and  display  similarities  in  a  number  of  aspects 
ranging  from  habitat  preference  and  morphology  to  behavior,  they 
exhibit  large  differences  in  social  organization  and  mating  system. 
The  evolutionary  divergence  in  the  social  behavior  and  mating  systems 
of  R*.  pol  ionotus  and  the  prairie  forms  of  fL.  maniculatus.  such  as 
_P.  m.  bairdi,  may  be  explained  through  examination  of  the  differences 
in  the  amount  of  and  distribution  of  suitable  habitat  for  these  taxa, 
and  differences  in  climate  in  the  present  day  distribution  of  these 
taxa.  Blair  (1950)  and  Smith  (1966)  have  described  the  factors  that 
apparently  have  led  to  the  existing  distribution  of  Ej.   pol ionotus.  and 
Its  separation  from  the  parental  species  P_«.  manicu  latus. 


The  present  geographic  relationships  of  these  two 
species  can  be  explained  if  we  assume  continuous 
distribution  of  maniculatus  across  the  coastal 
plain  in  Pleistocene  time.  This  distribution 
possibly,  but  not  necessarity,  might  have  been 
only  in  a  narrow  strip  along  the  Gulf  beaches. 
With  encroachment  of  the  Gulf  on  the  land  during 
Pleistocene  inter-glacial  stages,  there  was  the 
opportunity  for  a  part  of  this  population  to  be 
isolated  in  Florida,  for  parts  of  Florida  projected 
as  islands  during  these  periods  (see  Cooke,  1939). 
The  postulated  coastal-plain  population  of 
manicu latus  disappeared  eastward  of  Texas, 
effectively  isolating  the  Florida  population. 
(Blair,  1950,  p.  266) 


130 


Certain  soil  characteristics  appeared  to  be 
important  n  limiting  the  distribution  of  mice.   In 
relatively  undisturbed  habitats,  the  mice  occurred 
primarily  on  fine  sand....  Deposits  of  sorted  sands 
have  been  laid  down  in  several  ways  in  Florida 
(Laessle,  1958b).  Wind  was  an  important  agent  along 
the  beach  dunes.  The  action  of  water  was  important 
along  the  flood  plains  of  large  rivers,  the  shore- 
lines of  lakes  and  islands,  and  submerged  offshore 
bars.  Al I  areas  above  the  current  water  level  were 
at  one  time  part  of  the  Florida  shoreline.  As  the 
water  level  fell  during  glacial  periods,  numerous 
deposits  of  fine  textured  sand  were  gradually  exposed. 
Their  continuity  was  later  destroyed  by  erosion  (Alt 
and  Brooks,  1965).  These  deposits  and  their 
associated  vegetation  are  frequently  widely  spaced 
with  the  intervening  habitat  unsuitable  for  the  old- 
field  mouse.  These  interrupted  sand  deposits  are 
ecological  islands  for  this  species.  (Smith,  1966, 
p.  13-15) 

The  climate  in  Florida  would  have  been  much  cooler  during  the 
Pleistocene  glaciations  than  at  present,  and  may  have  been  at  least 
somewhat  similar  to  conditions  under  which  fL.  nu.  bairdi  exists 
presently.  Assuming  that  the  Pleistocene  prairie  forms  of  P_*. 
maniculatus  would  share  many  characteristics  with  present  day  prairie 
forms  of  P.*.  maniculatus,  one  may  hypothesize  that  many  of  the 
characteristics  of  the  species  ancestral  to  Pj.  pol iopotus  may 
presently  be  exhibited  by  fL_  nk  bairdi.  Among  these  characteristics 
would  be  the  ability  to  construct  shallow  burrows  (Houtcooper,  1972), 
tendency  to  form  winter  aggregations  (Howard,  1949)  and  to  cache  food 
for  winter  use  (Hamilton,  W.  J.,  1943;  Howard,  1949),  and  possibly  a 
predisposition  under  some  conditions  to  form  exclusive  reproductive 
pairs  (Howard,  1949). 

Food  caches  and  winter  aggregations  may  have  been  as  adaptive 
for  the  ancestral  stock  P_,_  pol  ionotus  was  derived  from  as  they  appear 
to  be  for  £*  nu.   bairdi  today  (Howard,  1951).   As  the  glaciers 


131 


retreated  and  the  climate  warmed,  however,  the  function  of  these  habits 
may  have  changed. •  The  loose  sand  soils  available  may  have  allowed  the 
ancestral  P_«.  pol  ionotus  to  construct  deeper  burrows  than  its 
predecessors.  This  factor,  and  a  warmer  climate,  would  have  allowed 
the  ancestral  species  to  maintain  a  favorable  temperature  in  nest 
burrows  year-round,  and  would  probably  have  resulted  in  lower 
mortality.  The  advent  of  more  constant  nest  conditions  would  have  in 
turn  reduced  the  necessity  for  large  winter  aggregations  as  it  would  be 
more  likely  that  a  family  unit  (parents  and  offspring)  could  maintain 
an  adequate  nest  temperature  alone.  More  constant  nest  burrow 
conditions  and  warmer  climate  would  also  act  to  reduce  the  need  for 
large  food  caches  for  winter  survival.  Food  caches  may,  however,  also 
serve  another  function.  Peromyscus  maniculatus.  although  normal ly  a 
seasonal  breeder,  is  capable  of  breeding  through  the  winter  if  adequate 
food  is  available  (Linduska,  1942;  Taitt,  1981).  Conditions  of  more 
moderate  and  stable  temperature,  along  with  an  increased  probability  of 
an  adequate  winter  food  supply,  are  likely  to  have  increased  the 
possibility  for  successful  year-round  breeding  In  fL.  pol ionotus.  The 
capacity  of  winter  breeding  would  further  act  to  limit  winter 
aggregations  to  immediate  family,  because  It  would  be  more  adaptive  to 
use  food  caches  to  produce  additional  offspring,  than  to  use  these 
resources  to  increase  Inclusive  fitness  through  supporting  more  distant 
rel atives. 

The  possibility  of  breeding  continuously,  in  conjunction  with 
ecological  factors,  may  have  provided  a  basis  for  the  establishment  of 
monogamy  as  the  predominant  mating  behavior  in  Pj.  pol ionotus.  The 


132 


major  ecological  factors  of  importance  to  monogamy  in  this  species,  as 
discussed  previously,  appear  to  be  the  availability  and  distribution  of 
nest  sites  and  food  items.  Constructing  deep  nest  burrows  limits 
choice  of  breeding  areas;  and  caching  of  food  suggests  that  food  may 
be  only  seasonally  abundant,  with  more  food  available  than  necessary 
for  survival  and  breeding  in  warmer  months,  and  a  reduced  food  supply 
in  winter  months.  Distribution  and  availability  of  nest  sites  and  food 
items  may  act,  as  discussed  earlier,  to  limit  possibilities  for 
polygamous  matings  by  males.  Constructing  deep  nest  burrows  and 
provisioning  food  caches,  however,  provide  a  stable  breeding 
environment  for  E*.   pol ionotus.  A  longer  breeding  season  would  allow 
females  to  produce  more  offspring.  Through  investment  in  burrows  and 
food  caches,  males  may  have  been  able  to  increase  the  number  of 
offspring  they  produced  by  pairing  with  a  single  female,  to  above  that 
they  would  have  expected  by  mating  polygamous ly.  Although  this  shift 
in  the  behavior  of  ancestral  £*  pol ionotus  males  could  be  interpreted 
as  "investment  in  offspring"  in  a  very  broad  sense,  these  behaviors  do 
not  really  go  beyond  those  presently  practiced  by  polygamous  Pj.  nu 
bairdi  males,  who  also  maintain  nests  and  cache  food  that  may  be  used 
by  mates  and  offspring.  The  major  shift  that  occurred  in  individuals 
of  the  ancestral  species  may  rather  be  interpreted  as  a  shift  in 
emphasis  from  behavior  resulting  in  increased  inclusive  fitness  through 
benefits  to  distant  relatives,  to  a  limitation  of  these  same  benefits 
to  offspring  and  to  increased  productivity  by  male-female  pairs.  This, 
of  course,  does  not  preclude  the  possibility  that  improved  male  care  of 
offspring  could  have  been  a  factor  that  added  to  the  adaptive  value  of 


133 


exclusive  breeding  relationships  in  E*.  pol ionotus.  but  suggests  that 
such  behavior  may  not  have  been  necessary  for  the  evolution  of  monogamy 
in  this  species. 

Summary 

The  present  study  is  consistent  with  the  hypothesis  that 
aggressive  ability  may  serve  as  a  basis  in  mate  selection  for  both 
sexes  in  monogamous  as  well  as  non-monogamous  species.   In  a 
seminatural  setting  aggressive  interactions  occurred  frequently  between 
members  of  both  sexes  of  the  monogamous  species  E*.  pol ionotus,  and 
individuals  within  groups  appeared  to  form  stable  aggressive 
relationships.  Males  of  this  species  exhibited  a  behavior,  aggressive 
digging,  that  may  function  to  signal  their  aggressive  status  to 
females.   Individuals  of  this  species  of  both  sexes  nested  more 
frequently  with  opposite-sexed  individuals  that  exhibited  high  rather 
than  low  aggressive  ability.  Male  and  female  E*.  pol ionotus.  and  male 
and  female  E*.  maniculatus.  also  exhibited  evidence  of  preference  for 
more  assertive  opposite-sexed  individuals  (high  rather  than  low 
tendency  to  interact)  when  tested  in  a  preference  apparatus. 

Preference  for  individuals  of  high  aggressive  ability  appears 
to  be  adaptive  in  terms  of  the  ecology  and  social  system  of  these  two 
species.   In  E*.  pol ionotus  high  aggressive  ability  may  insure  that 
an  individual  is  able  to  obtain  limited  nest  sites  and  food  and  defend 
them  against  conspeci f ics.  Female  aggression  could  also  be  a  factor 
acting  to  maintain  monogamy  in  this  species  (see:  Kleiman,  1977; 
Whittenberger,  1979,  1981;  Whittenberger  &  Tilson,  1980).  Females  of 
this  species,  however,  do  not  appear  to  be  dominant  over  males  (see 


134 


Smith,  1966).  Peromvscus  maniculatus  of  both  sexes  appear  to  utilize 
aggressive  ability  to  limit  settlement  of  juveniles  on  their  home  range 
(Ayer  &  Whitsett,  1980;  Enders,  1978;  Fordham,  1971;  Taitt,  1981; 
Whitsett  et  al.,  1979).  Aggressive  ability  is  also  important  in 
male-male  competition  in  E*.  maniculatus  (Blair  &  Howard,  1944; 
Dewsbury,  1981c).  Although  in  order  for  it  to  be  adaptive  to  choose 
mates  with  high  aggressive  ability  it  Is  not  necessary  for  aggressive 
ability  to  be  heritable,  the  adaptiveness  of  such  choice  would  be 
expected  to  increase  if  components  of  this  ability  were  heritable. 

In  preference  tests  familiarity  appeared  to  be  an  important 
factor  to  individuals  of  both  sexes  of  the  polygamous  species  E*. 
maniculatus.  but  of  little  consequence  to  individuals  of  either  sex  of 
the  monogamous  E*.  pol ionotus.  The  lack  of  significant  preference  for 
familiar  individuals  by  E*.  pol ionotus  in  preference  tests  was 
consistent  with  observations  of  the  nesting  behavior  of  this  species  in 
the  seminatural  apparatus.  Although  the  preferences  displayed  by  Et. 
maniculatus  could  be  a  result  of  housing  conditions  prior  to 
familiarity  tests  (Cam,  Krames  &  Costanzo,  1970;  Cam  et  al.,  1979; 
Cam  et  a  I.,  1980;  Krames  et  a  I.,  1967),  these  conditions  do  not 
appear  to  provide  an  explanation  for  the  lack  of  preference  displayed 
by  E*.  pol ionotus.  Differences  in  the  responses  of  P.  maniculatus  and 
E*.  pol ionotus  to  familiar  individuals  in  preference  tests  may  be  based 
on  differences  in  the  opportunities  individuals  of  these  two  species 
have  to  make  use  of  this  factor  in  mate  selection.  As  a  result  of 
factors  of  ecology,  social  behavior,  and  breeding  system,  these 
opportunities  may  be  much  more  limited  for  £j,  pol ionotus  than  for  Ex 


135 


maniculatus..  Familiarity  may,  however,  aid  in  maintaining  pair  bonds 
in  P.  pol ionotus  through  reducing  aggression,  as  familiarity  did 
appear  to  reduce  aggression  between  familiar  opposite-sexed  individuals 
in  seminatural  experiments. 

Although  only  E*  pol ionotus  females  demonstrated  a  significant 
preference  for  siblings  over  nonsiblings,  males  of  this  species  also 
tended  to  display  higher  sibling  than  nonsibling  scores  in  preference 
tests.  This  finding  is  consistent  with  the  observation  by  Smith  (1966) 
that  female  E*  pol ionotus  appear  to  prefer  siblings  as  mates  over 
nonsiblings.  Peromyscus  maniculatus  of  both  sexes  displayed  only 
nonsignif icantly  higher  scores  for  nonsiblings  than  for  siblings  in 
preference  tests. 

Inbreeding  in  Ex  pol ionotus  may  be  an  adaptive  strategy  that 
permits  individuals  of  this  species  to  found  populations  in  Isolated 
patches  of  favorable  habitat.  A  similar  strategy  has  previously  been 
proposed  for  Microtus  pennsy I vanicus  (Batzli  et  al.,  1977;  Getz, 
1978).  The  lack  of  significant  preference  for  nonsiblings  demonstrated 
by  Ej,  maniculatus.  a  polygamous  species,  may  be  due  to  competing 
preference  responses  in  this  species.  Although  P^  maniculatus  appear 
to  avoid  breeding  with  siblings  (Hill,  1974;  Dewsbury,  1982a), 
Individuals  may  also  be  attracted  to  relatives  through  a  preference  for 
mating  on  or  near  their  natal  home  range,  and  through  opportunities  to 
increase  their  inclusive  fitness  through  interactions  with  relatives. 

The  shift  from  polygamy  to  monogamy  in  ancestral  E*.  pol ionotus 
may  have  occurred  as  a  result  of  a  shift  from  an  emphasis  on 
aggressively  limiting  settlement  on  home  ranges  to  defense  of  the 
nest  site,  concomitant  with  a  shift  away  from  increasing  inclusive 


136 


fitness  through  aid  to  distant  relatives  to  increasing  personal 
fitness  through  limiting  aid  to  a  single  mate. 


REFERENCES 


Adler,  N.  T.  On  the  mechanisms  of  sexual  behavior  and  their 
evolutionary  constraints.   In  J.  B.  Hutchison  (Ed.)  Biological 
determinants  o±   sexual  behavior.   New  York:  Wiley,  1978. 


Agren,  G.  Social  and  territorial  behaviour  in  the  mongolian  gerbi 
(Meriones  ungu  icu latus)  under  seminatural  conditions.  Biology, 
of  Behaviour.  1976,  1,  267-285. 


Agren,  6.,  &  Meyerson,  B.  J.   Influence  of  gonadal  hormones  on  the 
behaviour  of  pair- I iving  mongol  ian  gerbi Is  (Meriones  unguicu latus) 
towards  the  cagemate  versus  a  non-cagemate  in  a  social  choice  test. 
Behavioural  Processes.  1977,  2,   325-335. 

Alexander,  R.  0.  The  evolution  of  social  behavior.  Annual  Review  oi 
Ecology  and  System ics.  1974,  5_,  325-383. 

Alexander,  R.  D.  Natural  selection  and  specialized  chorusing  behavior 
in  acoustical  insects.   In  D.  Pimentel  (Ed.)  Insects,  science, 
and  society.  New  York:  Academic  Press,  1975. 

All  in,  J.  T.,  &  Banks,  E.  M.   Behavioral  biology  of  the  collared  lemming 
Dicrostonvx  nroen I  and icus  (Traill):   I.  agonistic  behavior.   Animal 
-Behavior,  1968,  16,  245-262. 


Alt,  D.,  &  Brooks,  H.  K.  Age  of  the  Florida  marine  terraces.   Journal 
of  Geology.  1965,  73,  406-411. 

Armstrong,  E.  A.  A  study  of  birdsong.  London:  Dover,  1973. 

Ayer,  M.  L.,  &  Whitsett,  J.  M.  Aggressive  behavior  of  female  prairie 
deermice  in  laboratory  populations.   Animal  Behaviour.  1980,  28., 
163-71}. 


137 


138 


Baker,  R.  H.  Habitats  and  distribution.   In  J.  A.  King  (Ed.)  Biology 
0±   Peromyscus  ( Rodent i a) .  Lawrence,  Kansas:  American  Society 
of  Mammalogists,  1968. 

Barnett,  S.  A.,  Evans,  C.  S.,  &  Stoddart,  R.  C.   Influence  of  females  on 
conflict  among  wild  rats.  Journal  of  Zoology.  1968,  154,  391-396. 

Bateman,  A.  J.   Intra-sexual  selection  in  Drosophilia.  Heredity,  1948, 
Z,   349-368. 

Bateson,  P.  Sexual  imprinting  and  optimal  outbreeding.  Nature,  1978, 
273.  659-660. 

Bateson,  P.  How  do  sensitive  periods  arise  and  what  are  they  for? 
Animal  Behaviour.  1979,  27,  470-486. 


Bateson,  P.  Optimal  outbreeding  and  the  development  of  sexual 
preferences  in  Japanese  quail.  Zeitschrift  ±ul   Tierpsychologv. 
1980,  51,  231-244. 

Batzli,  G.  0.,  Getz,  L.  L.,  &  Hurley,  S.  S.  Suppression  of  growth  and 
reproduction  of  microtine  rodents  by  social  factors.   Journal  oj. 
Mammalogy.  1977,  5_£,  583-591. 

Bediz,  G.  M. ,  &  Whitsett,  J.  M.  Social  inhibition  of  sexual  maturation 
in  male  prairie  deer  mice.  Journal  of  Comparative  and.  Physiological 
Psychology.  1979,  21,  493-500. 

Bengtsson,  B.  0.   Avoid  inbreeding:  At  what  cost?  Journal  of_ 
Theoretical  Biology.  1978,  21,  439-444. 


Bertram,  B.  C.  R.  Social  factors  influencing  reproduction  in  wild 
lions.   Journal  of  Zoology.  1975,  177,  463-482. 

Bertram,  B.  C.  R.  Kin  selection  in  lions  and  in  evolution.   In  P.  P.  G. 
Bateson  &  R.  A.  Hinde  (Eds.)  Growing  points  In  ethology.   Cambridge: 
Cambridge  University  Press,  1976. 

Birdsall,  D.  A.,  &  Nash,  D.  Occurrence  of  successful  multiple 
insemination  of  females  in  natural  populations  of  deer  mice 
(Peromyscus  manicul  atus) .   Evolution.  1973,  27.,  106-110. 


139 


Bixler,  R.  H.  The  incest  controversy.  Psychological  Reports,  1981, 
42,  267-283. 


Hair,  W.  F.  A  study  of  prairie  deer-mouse  populations  in  southern 
Michigan.   American  Midland  Naturalist.  1940,  24,  273-305. 


lair,  W.  F.   Size  of  home  range  and  notes  on  the  life  history  of  the 
woodland  deer-mouse  and  eastern  chipmunk  in  northern  Michigan. 
Journal  oi   Mammalogy,  1942,  23_,  27-36. 


ilair,  W.  F.  Populations  of  the  deer-mouse  and  associated  small  mammals 
in  the  mesquite  association  of  southern  New  Mexico.  Contributions 
from  the  Laboratory  of  Vertebrate  Biology,  University  of  Michigan,  Ann 
Arbor,  1943,  2JL,  1-40. 


ilair,  W.  F.  Ecological  factors  in  the  speciation  of  Peromyscus. 

Evolution.  1950,  4,  253-275. 

Hair,  W.  F.  Population  structure,  social  behavior,  and  environmental 
relations  in  a  natural  population  of  the  beach  mouse  (Peromyscus 

pol ionotus  leucocephalus) .  Contributions  from  the  Laboratory  of 

Vertebrate  Biology.   University  of  Michigan,  Ann  Arbor,  1951,  4_8_, 
1-47. 


Blair,  W.  F.  Effects  of  x-irradiation  on  a  natural  population  of  the 
deer  mouse  (Peromyscus  man  icu  I  atus) .  Ecology,  1958,  3_9_,  113-118. 


Blair,  W.  F.,  &  Howard,  W.  E.  Experimental  evidence  of  sexual  isolation 
between  three  forms  of  the  cenospecies  Peromyscus  man icu I atus. 
Contributions  from  the  Laboratory  of  Vertebrate  Biology,  University 
of  Michigan,  Ann  Arbor,  Ml,  1944,  2£,  1-19. 


Boice,  R.  Burrows  of  wild  and  albino  rats:  Effects  of  domestication, 
outdoor  raising,  age,  experience  and  maternal  state.   Journal  of 
Comparative  and  Physiological  Psychology,  1977,  91,  649-661. 

Boice,  R.,  &  Adams,  N.  Outdoor  enclosures  for  feral  izing  rats  and  mice. 
Behavior  Research  Methods  &  Instrumentation,  1980,  12,  577-582. 

Borgia,  G.   Sexual  selection  and  the  evolution  of  mating  systems.   In 
M.  S.  Blum  &  N.  A.  Blum  (Eds.)   Sexual  selection  and  reproductive 
competition  in  insects.   New  York:   Academic  Press  Inc.,  1979. 


140 


Bowen,  D.  W.,  &  Brooks,  R.  J.   Social  organization  of  confined  male 
collared  lemmings  (Dicrostonvx  groen I  and icus  Traill).  Animal 
Behaviour,  1978,  2£_,  1126-1135. 


Jrain,  P.  F.,  Benton,  D.,  &  Bolton,  J.  C.  Comparison  of  agonistic 
behavior  in  individually  housed  male  mice  with  those  cohabiting 
with  females.   Aggressive  Behavior.  1978,  4,  201-206. 


Briese,  L.  A.,  &  Smith,  M.  H.  Competition  between  Mus  musculus  and 
Peromyscus  pol  ionotus.   Journal  of  Mammalogy,  1973,  5_4,  968-969. 


Brown,  J.  L.  The  evolution  of  diversity  in  avian  territorial  systems. 
Wilson  Bulletin.  1964,  7£,  161-169. 


Brown,  J.  L.  JM   evolution  oj.  behavior.   New  York:  W.  W.  Norton 
Company,  Inc.  1975. 


Brown,  R.  E.  Mammalian  social  odors:  A  critical  review.   In 
Advances  in  ihe   study  o±   behavior  (Vol.  10).  New  York: 
Academic  Press,  1979. 


Bruce,  H.  M.  An  exteroceptive  block  to  pregnancy  in  the  mouse. 
Nature.  1959,  181,  105. 


Bruce,  H.  M.  A  block  to  pregnancy  in  the  mouse  caused  by  the 
proximity  of  strange  males.   Journal  of  Reproduction  and 
Fertility.  1960,  1,  96-103. 


Bulmer,  M.  G.   Inbreeding  in  the  great  tit.  Heredity.  1973,  3_0_, 
313-325. 


Burley,  N.  Parental  investment,  mate  choice,  and  mate  quality. 
Proceedings  of  the  National  Academy  of  Science,  1977,  2A, 
3476-3479. 


Burley,  N.  Mate  choice  by  multiple  criteria  in  a  monogamous  species. 
American  Natural  1st.  1981,  111,  515-528. 


Caldwell,  L.  D.,  &  Gentry,  J.  B.   Interactions  of  Peromyscus  and  Mus. 
in  a  one-acre  field  enclosure.   Ecology,  1965,  46,  189-192.  (a) 


141 


Caldwell,  L.  D.,  &  Gentry,  J.  B.  Natality  in  Peromyscus  pol ionotus 
populations.  American  Midland  Naturalist,  1965,  21,  168-175.  (b) 


Carmichael,  M.  S.  Sexual  discrimination  by  golden  hamsters 

(Mesocricetus  auratus) .  Behavioral  and  Neural  Biology,  1980,  22., 
73-90.  '   


Carr,  W.  J.,  Demesqu ita-Wander,  M. ,  Sachs,  S.  R. ,  &  Macon  J,  P. 
Responses  of  female  rats  to  odors  from  familiar  vs.  novel  males. 
Bulletin  of  the  Psychonomic  Society.  1979,  1A,  118-1120. 


Carr,  W.  J.,  Hirsch,  J.  T. ,  &  Balazs,  J.  M.  Responses  of  male  rats  to 
odors  from  familiar  vs.  novel  females.  Behavioral  and  Neural 
Biology.  1980,  29,  331-337. 


Carr,  W.  J.,  Krames,  L.,  &  Costanzo,  D.  J.  Previous  sexual  experience 
and  olfactory  preference  for  novel  versus  original  sex  partners 
i  n  rats .   Journal  of  Comparative  and  Physiological  Psychology, 
1970,  71,  216-222.  ~"   "" 


Carr,  W.  J.,  Martorano,  R.  D.,  &  Krames,  L.  Responses  of  mice  to  odors 
associated  with  stress.   Journal  of  Comparative  and  Physiological 
Psychology,  1970,  71,  223-228.        '""'  '~   ~ " 


Carr,  W.  J.,  Wylie,  N.  R. ,  &  Loeb,  L.  S.   Responses  of  adult  and 
immature  rats  to  sex  odors.   Journal  of  Comparative  and 
Physiological  Psychology,  1970,  22,  51-59. 


Clutton-Brock,  T.  H.,  &  Harvey,  P.  H.  Primate  ecology  and  social 
organization.  Journal  of  Zoology,  1977,  183.  1-39. 


Clutton-Brock,  T.  H.,  &  Harvey,  P.  H.   Mammals,  resources  and 
reproductive  strategies.  Nature,  1978,  273.  191-195. 


Clutton-Brock,  T.  H.,  &  Albon,  S.  D.  The  roaring  of  red  deer  and 
the  evolution  of  honest  advertisement.   Behaviour.  1979, 
GS,  145-170. 


Clutton-Brock,  T.  H.,  Albon,  S.  D.,  Gibson,  R.  M. ,  &  Guinness, 
F.  E.  The  logical  stag:   Adaptive  aspects  of  fighting  in  red 
deer  (Cervus  el  aphus  L. ) .  Animal  Behaviour,  1979,  22.,  211-225. 


142 


Cogshall,  A.  S.  Food  habits  of  deer  mice  of  the  genus  Peromyscus  in 
captivity.  Journal  of  Mammalogy.  1928,  9_,  217-221. 

Colvin,  D.  V.  Agonistic  behavior  in  males  of  five  species  of  voles 
Microtus.   Animal  Behavior.  1973,  21,  471-480. 

Cooke,  C.  W.  Scenery  of  Florida  interpreted  by  a  geologist.  Florida 
Geological  Bulletin.  1939,  12,  1-118. 

Costanzo,  D.  J.,  &  Renfrew,  J.  Preference  of  female  rats  for  odors  of 
dominant  versus  subordinate  males.  Paper  presented  at  the  meetings 
of  the  Psychonomic  Society,  Washington,  D.C.  1977. 

Coulson,  J.  C.  The  influence  of  the  pair-bond  and  age  on  the  breeding 
biology  of  the  kittiwake  gul I  Risa  tridactvla.   Journal  of  Animal 
Ecology,  1966,  3_5_,  269-279. 

Cowan,  D.  P.  Sibling  mating  in  a  hunting  wasp:  Adaptive  inbreeding? 
Science.  1979,  201,    1403-1405. 

Cox,  C.  R.  Agonistic  encounters  among  male  elephant  seals:  Frequency, 
context,  and  the  role  of  female  preference.  American  Zoologist, 
1981,  21,  197-209. 

Cox,  C.  R. ,  &  LeBoeuf,  B.  J.  Female  incitation  of  male  competition: 
A  mechanism  in  sexual  selection.  American  Natural ist,  1977,  HI, 
317-335. 


Crook,  J.  H.  On  the  integration  of  gender  strategies  in  mammalian 
social  systems.   In  J.  S.  Rosenblatt  &  B.  R.  Komisaruk  (Eds.) 
Reproductive  behavior  and  evolution.  New  York:  Plenum  Press,  1977. 

Crowcroft,  P.,  &  Rowe,  F.  P.  Social  organization  and  territorial 
behavior  in  the  wild  house  mouse  (Mus  musculus  L.) .  Proceedings 
o_f_  the  Zoological  Society  of  London_,  1963,  140.,  517-531. 

Daly,  M.,  &  Wilson,  M.   Sex,  evolution  &  behavior.   North  Scituate,  MA: 
Duxbury  Press,  1978. 

Dapson,  R.  W.  Age  structure  of  six  populations  of  old-field  mice, 
Peromyscus  polionotus.   Research  in  Population  Ecology,  1972, 
13_,  161-169. 


143 


Darwin,  C.  The  origin  of  species.  London:  John  Murray,  1859. 


Darwin,  C.  The  descent  of  man;  And  selection  in  relation  to  sex. 
Second  ed.,  London:  John  Murray,  1874. 


Davenport,  L.  B.,  Jr.  Structure  of  two  Peromyscus  pol ionotus 
populations  in  old-field  ecosystems  at  the  AEC  Savannah  River 
Plant.   Journal  of  Mammalogy.  1964,  45_,  95-113. 


Davis,  J.  IV.  F.,  &  O'Donald,  P.  Territory  size,  breeding  time,  and 
mating  preference  in  the  Arctic  skua.  Nature.  1976,  260.  774-775. 


Dawkins,  R.  J_he  selfish  gene.  Oxford:   Oxford  University  Press,  1976. 


de  Catanzero,  D.  Facilitation  of  intermale  aggression  in  mice  through 
exposure  to  receptive  females.   Journal  of  Comparative  and 
Physiological  Psychology.  1981,  9_5_,  638-645. 


DeJonge,  G.  Response  to  con  and  heterospeci f ic  male  odors  by  the  voles 
Microtus  agrestis.  M.  arval is  and  Clethr ionomys  glareol us  with  respect 
to  competition  for  space.   Behaviour.  1980,  73_,  277-303. 


Dewsbury,  D.  A.   The  use  of  muroid  rodents  in  the  psychology  laboratory. 
Behavior  Research  Methods  &  I  nstrumentation,  1974,  6,  301-308. 


Dewsbury,  D.  A.  The  comparative  method  in  studies  of  reproductive 
behavior.   In  T.  E.  McGill,  D.  A.  Dewsbury,  &  B.  D.  Sachs  (Eds.) 
Sex  and  Behavior.   New  York:   Plenum  Publishing  Corporation,  1978. 


Dewsbury,  D.  A.  Copulatory  behavior  of  deer  mice  (Peromyscus 

manicul atus) :   II.   A  study  of  some  factors  regulating  the  fine 
structure  of  behavior.  Journal  of  Comparative  and  Physiological 
Psychology.  1979,  93,  161-177. 


Dewsbury,  D.  A.   An  exercise  in  the  prediction  of  monogamy  in  the  field 
from  laboratory  data  on  42  species  of  muroid  rodents.   Biologist. 
1981,  63,  138-162.   (a) 


Dewsbury,  D.  A.   Effects  of  novelty  on  copulatory  behavior:  The 
Coolidge  effect  and  related  phenomena.  Psychological  Bui  let  in. 
1981 ,  29,  464-482.   (b) 


144 


Dewsbury,  D.  A.  Social  dominance,  copulatory  behavior,  and  differential 
reproduction  in  deer  mice  (Peromyscus  maniculatus) .  Journal  o± 
Comparative  and  Physiological  Psychology.  1981,  9J5,  880-095.   (c) 


Dewsbury,  D.  A.  Avoidance  of  incestuous  breeding  between  siblings  in 
two  species  of  Peromyscus  mice.  Biology  of  Behavior.  1982,  7, 
157-169.   (a) 


Dewsbury,  D.  A.  Dominance  rank,  copulatory  behavior,  and  differential 
reproduction.  Quarterly  Review  of  Biology.  1982,  5J7_,  135-159.   (b) 


Dewsbury,  D.  A.  Ejaculate  cost  and  male  choice.  American  Natural ist. 
1982,  H9_,  601-610.   (c) 


Dewsbury,  D.  A.,  &  Baumgardner,  D.  J.   Studies  of  sperm  competition  in 
two  species  of  muroid  rodents.  Behavioral  Ecology  and  Sociob ioloav. 
1981,  £,  121-133. 

Dewsbury,  D.  A.,  &  Rethl ingshaf er,  D.  A.   Comparative  psychology;  A 
modern  survey.  New  York:   McGraw-Hill,  1973. 


Dice,  L.  R.  Some  factors  affecting  the  distribution  of  the  prairie 
vole,  forest  deer  mouse,  and  prairie  deer  mouse.  Ecology.  1922, 
3_,  29-47. 


Dice,  L.  R.,  &  Howard,  W.  E.  Distance  of  dispersal  by  prairie  deermice 
from  birthplaces  to  breeding  sites.   Contributions  from  the 
Laboratory  of  Vertebrate  Biology.  University  of  Michigan,  Ann 
Arbor,  Ml,  1951,  50,  1-15. 


Dienske,  H.  The  importance  of  social  interactions  and  habitat  in 
competition  between  Microtus  agresti  s  and  H*.  arval i  s.  Behaviour. 
1979,  71,  1-126. 


Drickamer,  L.  C.   Annual  reproduction  patterns  in  populations  of  two 
sympatric  species  of  Peromyscus.   Behavioral  Biology.  1978,  23_, 
405-408. 


Drickamer,  L.  C.   Acceleration  and  delay  of  first  estrus  in  wild  Mus 
muscul us.   Journal  of  Mammalogy.  1979,  60,  215-216. 


145 


Elsenberg,  J.  F.  Studies  on  the  behavior  of  Peromyscus  maniculatus 
qambel  i  i  and  Peromyscus  californicus  parasiticus.  Behaviour,  1962, 
19_,  177-207. 


Eisenberg,  J.  F.  The  evolution  of  the  reproductive  unit  in  the  class 
mammalia.   In  J.  S.  Rosenblatt  &  B.  R.  Komisaruk  (Eds.)  Reproductive 
behavior  and  evolution.  New  York:  Plenum  Publishing  Corporation, 
1977. 


Emlen,  S.  T.  The  evolution  of  helping.   I.  An  ecological  constraints 
model.  American  Natural  ist.  1982,  119_,  29-53. 

Emlen,  S.  T. ,  &  Oring,  L.  W.  Ecology,  sexual  selection,  and  the 
evolution  of  mating  systems.  Science,  1977,  12Z,  215-223. 

Enders,  J.  E.  Social  interactions  between  confined  juvenile  and  adult 
Peromyscus  maniculatus  bairdi  i:   Effects  of  social  factors  on 
juvenile  settlement  and  growth.   (Doctoral  dissertation,  Michigan 
State  University,  1977).  Dissertation  Abstracts  International,  1978, 
3_8_,  4680B-4681B.   (University  Microfilms  No.  780341) 

Epple,  G.  The  role  of  pheromones  in  the  social  communication  of 

marmoset  monkeys.   Journal  of  Reproduction  and  Fertility  Supplement, 
1973,  19.,  447-454. 

Epple,  G.  Olfactory  communication  in  South  American  primates.  Annals 
N.Y.  Academy  q±   Science.  1974,  221,  261-278. 

Erickson,  C.  J.,  &  Zenone,  P.  G.  Courtship  differences  in  male  ring 
doves:  Avoidance  of  cuckoldry?  Science.  1976,  122.,  1353-1354. 

Fairbairn,  D.  J.  Why  breed  early?  A  study  of  reproductive  tactics  in 
Peromyscus.  Canadian  Journal  of  Zoology,  1977,  5JL,  862-871. 

Fairbairn,  D.  J.  Behavior  of  dispersing  deer  mice  (Peromyscus 

maniculatus) .  Behavioral  Ecology  and  Sociobioloqy,  1978,  3_,  265-282. 

Farr,  L.  A.,  Andrews,  R.  V.,  &  Kline,  M.  R.   Comparison  of  methods  for 
estimating  social  rank  of  deer  mice.  Behavioral  Biology,  1978,  23_, 
399-404. 


146 


Fass,  B.,  Guterman,  P.  E.,  &  Stevens,  D.  A.  Evidence  that  rats 
discriminate  between  familiar  and  unfamiliar  putative  urinary 
oderants  of  adult  male  conspecif ics.  Aggressive  Behavior,  1978, 
4,  231-236. 


Fisher,  R.  A.  The  qenetical  theory  of  natural  selection.  New  York: 
Dover,  1958. 


Flannel  iy,  K.,  &  Lore,  R.  The  influence  of  females  upon  aggression  in 
domesticated  male  rats  (Rattus  norvegicus).  Anjmal  Behaviour,  1977, 
21,   651-659. 


Foltz,  D.  W.  Genetics  and  mating  system  of  the  oldfield  mouse 
(Peromyscus  polionotus).   (Doctoral  dissertation,  University  of 
Michigan,  1979).  Dissertation  Abstracts  International,  1980,  4J1, 
4692B-4693B.   (University  Microfilms  No.  8007739) 


Foltz,  D.  W.  Genetic  evidence  for  long-term  monogamy  in  a  small 
rodent,  Peromyscus  polionotus.  American  Natural  ist,  1981,  1 17> 
665-675.  (a) 


Foltz,  D.  W.  Genetic  measures  of  inbreeding  in  Peromyscus.   Journal 
of  Mammalogy.  1981,  £2,  470-476.  (b) 


Fordham,  R.  A.  Field  populations  of  deermice  with  supplemental  food. 
Ecology.  1972,  52,  138-146. 


Garson,  P.  J.  Social  interactions  of  woodmice  (Apodemus  syl vaticus) 
studied  by  direct  observation  in  the  wild.   Journal  of  Zoology.  1975, 
177 f  496-500. 


Garten,  C.  T. ,  Jr.  Relationships  between  aggressive  behavior  and  genie 
heterozygosity  in  the  oldfield  mouse  Peromyscus  pol ionotus. 
Evolution,  1976,  3_0_,  59-72. 


Garten,  C.  T. ,  Jr.  Relationships  between  exploratory  behaviour  and  genie 
heterozygosity  in  the  oldfield  mouse.  Animal  Behaviour,  1977, 
21,   328-332. 


Gashwiler,  J.  S.  Deer  mouse  reproduction  and  its  relationship  to  the 
tree  seed  crop.   American  Midland  Natural ist.  1979,  1Q2,  95-104. 


147 


Gentry,  J.  B.   Invasion  of  a  one-year  abandoned  field  by  Peromyscus 
polionotus  and  Mus  musculus.  Journal  of  Mammalogy,  1966,  41,  431-439. 

Gentry,  J.  B. ,  &  Smith,  M.  H.  Food  habits  and  burrow  associates  of 
Peromyscus  polionotus.   Journal  of  Mammalogy,  1968,  4_9_,  562-565. 

Getz.  L.  L.  Speculation  on  social  structure  and  population  cycles  of 
microtine  rodents.  The  Biologist.  1978,  60,  134-147. 

Getz,  L.  L.,  &  Carter,  C.  S.  Social  organization  in  Mjcrptus 
oohrogaster  populations.  Ihje  Biologist.  1980,  62,  56-69. 

Getz,  L.  L.,  Carter,  C.  S.,  &  Gavish,  L.  The  mating  system  of  the  prairie 
vole,  Microtus  ochrogaster:  Field  and  laboratory  evidence  for 
pair-bonding.  Behavioral  Ecology  and  Soc iobiology,  1981,  8,  189-194. 

Gilder,  P.  M.,  &  Slater,  P.  J.  B.   Interest  of  mice  in  conspecific 
male  odors  is  influenced  by  degree  of  kinship.  Nature,  1978,  2JA, 
364-365. 

Gipps,  J.  H.,  &  Jewell,  P.  A.  Maintaining  populations  of  bank  voles, 
niethrionomvs  glareolus.  in  large  outdoor  enclosures,  and  measuring 
the  response  of  population  variables  to  the  castration  of  males. 
Journal  of  Animal  Ecology.  1979,  4J5,  535-555. 

Goforth,  W.,  &   Baskett,  T.  Social  organization  of  penned  Mourning 
doves.   Aiik,  1971,  S£,  528-542. 

Golley,  F.  B. ,  Gentry,  J.  B.,  Caldwell,  L.  D.,  &  Davenport, _L.  B.,  Jr. 
Number  and  variety  of  small  mammals  on  the  AEC  Savannah  River  Plant. 
Journal  of  Mammalogy.  1965,  46,  1-18. 

Grafen,  A.,  &  Sibly,  R.  A  model  of  mate  desertion.  Animal  Behaviour, 
1978,  2£,  645-652. 

Grau,  H.  J.  Kin  recognition  in  white  footed  deer-mice  (Peromyscus 
leucopus).  Animal  Behavior.  1982,  20,  497-505. 

Halliday,  T.  R.   Sexual  selection  and  mate  choice.   In  J.  R.  Krebs 
&  N.  B.  Davies  (Eds.)   Behavioral  ecology:   An  evolutionary  approach. 
Oxford:  Blackwell  Scienti f ic  Pub  I ications,  1978. 


148 


Halpin,  Z.  T.   Individual  odors  and  individual  recognition:  Review  and 
commentary.   Biology  of  Behaviour.  1980,  5_,  233-248. 

Halpin,  Z.  T. ,  &  Hoffman,  M.  Kin  recognition  and  preference  in  the 
white-footed  mouse  (Peromyscus  leucopus):  Green  beard  or  early 
socialization?  Paper  presented  at  meetings  of  the  Animal  Behavior 
Society,  Knoxville,  Tennessee,  1981. 

Hamilton,  W.  D.  The  genetical  theory  of  social  behavior.   I.  Journal 
nf  Theoretical  Biology.  1964,  I,  1-16.  (a) 

Hamilton,  W.  D.  The  genetical  theory  of  social  behavior.   II.  Journal 
nf  Theoretical  Biology.  1964,  I,  17-52.  (b) 

Hamilton,  W.  D.,  &  Zuk,  M.  Heritable  true  fitness  and  bright  birds:  A 
role  for  parasites?  Science.  1982,  218.,  384-386. 

Hamilton,  W.  J.  The  mammals  of  eastern  United,  Slater  New  York: 
Comstock  Publishing  Co.,  1943. 

Hansen,  R.  M.  Communal  litters  of  Peromyscus  mgnjculatus.   Journal  of_ 
Mammalogy,  1957,  2£,  523. 

Harris,  V.  T.  An  experimental  study  of  habitat  selection  by  prairie 
and  forest  races  of  the  deermouse,  Peromyscus  manicu latus. 
Contributions  frnm_ th a  I  aboratory  of  Vertebrate  Biology, 
University  of  Michigan,  Ann  Arbor,  1952,  5JL,  1-53. 

Hayne,  D.  W.  Burrowing  habits  of  Peromyscus  pol  ionotus.   Jo_unnaJ  _oi 
Mammalogy,  1936,  II,  420-421. 

Healey,  M.  C.  Aggression  and  self-regulation  of  population  size  in 
deermice.   Ecology.  1967,  4&,  377-392. 

Heisler,  I.  L.  Offspring  quality  and  the  polygyny  threshold:   A  new 
model  for  the  "sexy  son"  hypothesis.   American  Natural ist, 
1981,  112,  316-327. 

Hill,  J.  L.  Peromyscus:  Effect  of  early  pairing  on  reproduction. 
Science.  1974,  1M,  1042-1044. 


149 


Hill,  J.  L.  Space  utilization  of  Peromyscus:  Social  and  spatial 
factors.   Animal  Behaviour.  1977,  21,   373-389. 


Hinde,  R.  A.  Animal  behavior:  A  synthesis  of  ethology  and  comparative 
Psychology.  2nd  ed.   New  York:   McGraw  Hill,  1966. 

Hogan-Warburg,  A.  J.  Social  behavior  of  the  ruff,  Phi lomachus  pugnax 
(L.).   Ardea.  1966,  51,  109-225. 


Holbrook,  S.  J.  Habitat  relationships  and  coexistence  of  four  sympatric 
species  of  peromyscus  in  northwestern  New  Mexico.   Journal  o_f 
Mammalogy.  1978,  52,  18-26. 

Hoogland,  J.  L.  Prairie  dogs  avoid  extreme  inbreeding.  Science,  1982, 
215,  1639-1641. 

Hooper,  E.  T.  Classification.   In  J.  A.  King  (Ed.)  Biology  o_f_ 
Peromyscus  ( Rodent i a) .  Lawrence,  Kansas:  American  Society  of 
Mammalogists,  1968. 

Houtcooper,  W.  C.  Rodent  seed  supply  and  burrows  of  Peromyscus  in 
cultivated  fields.  Proceedings  of  the  Indiana  Academy  o±  Science, 
1972,  81,  384-389. 


Houtcooper,  W.  C.  Food  habits  of  rodents  in  a  cultivated  ecosystem. 
Journal  of  Mammalogy.  1978,  52,  427-430. 


Howard,  W.  E.  Dispersal,  amount  of  inbreeding,  and  longevity  in  a 
local  population  of  prairie  deermice  on  the  George  Reserve,  Southern 
Michigan.  Contributions  from  the  Laboratory  of  Vertebrate  Biology, 
University  of  Michigan,  Ann  Arbor,  1949,  43,  1-52. 


Howard,  W.  E.  Relation  between  low  temperature  and  available  food  to 
survival  of  small  rodents.   Journal  pf  Mammalogy.  1951,  3.2,  300-312. 


Hrdy,  S.   Infanticide  among  animals:  a  review,  classification,  and 
examination  of  the  implications  for  the  reproductive  strategies  of 
females.  Ethology  and  Sociobiology.  1979,  1,  13-40. 


Huck,  U.  W.  Pregnancy  block  in  laboratory  mice  as  a  function  of  male 
social  status.   Journal  of  Reproduction  and  FertilityP  1982,  6_6_,  1 81  - 
184. 


150 


Huck,  U.  W.,  &  Banks,  E.  M.  Behavioral  components  of  individual 
recognition  in  the  collared  lemming  (Dicrostonyx  qroenlandicus) . 
Behavioral  Ecology  and.  Sociob  iology,  1979,  6_,  85-90. 

Huck,  U.  W.,  &  Banks,  E.  M.  The  effects  of  cross-fostering  on  the 
behaviour  of  two  species  of  North  American  lemmings,  Dicrogtonyx 
groenlandicus  and  Lemmus  trlmucronatus:   I.  olfactory  preferences. 
Animal  Behaviour.  1980,  28.,  1046-1052. 

Huck,  U.  W.,  &  Banks,  E.  M.  Male  dominance  status,  female  choice  and 
mating  success  in  the  brown  lemming,  Lemmus  tr imucropatus.  Animal 
Behaviour,  1982,  2fi,  665-675. 

Huck,  U.  W.,  Banks,  E.  M.,  &  Wang,  S.  Olfactory  discrimination  of 
social  status  in  the  brown  lemming.  Behavioral  and  Neural  Biology, 
1981,  51,  364-371. 

Huck,  U.  W.,  Soltis,  R.  L.,  &  Coopersmith,  C.  B.   Infanticide  in  male 
laboratory  mice:  Effects  of  social  status,  prior  sexual  experience, 
and  basis  for  discrimination  between  related  and  unrelated  young. 
Animal  Behaviour.  1982,  3£,  1158-1165. 

Jannett,  F.  J.,  Jr.  Social  dynamics  of  the  montane  vole,  Mjcrotus 
ochrogaster.  as  a  paradigm.  The  Biologist.  1980,  £2,  3-19. 

Jones,  R.  B. ,  &  Nowell,  N.  W.  Aversive  and  aggression  promoting 

properties  of  urine  from  dominant  and  subordiante  male  mice.  Animal 
Learning  aM  Behavior.  1973,  3_,  207-210. 

King,  J.  A.  Ecological  Psychology:  An  approach  to  motivation. 
Nebraska  Symposium  Motivation.  1970,  11,  1-33. 

Kleiman,  D.  G.  Monogamy  in  mammals.  Quarterly  Review  M  Biology,  1977, 
52,  36-69. 

Kleiman,  D.  G. ,  &  Malcolm,  J.  R.   The  evolution  of  male  parental 
investment  in  mammals.   In  D.  J.  Gubernick  &  P.  H.  Klopfer  (Eds.) 
Parental  care  in  mammals.   New  York:   Plenum  Pub  I ishing 
Corporation,  1  981  . 


151 


Koenlg,  W.  D.,  &  Pitelka,  F.  A.  Ecological  factors  and  kin  selection  in 
the  evolution  of  cooperative  breeding  in  birds.   In  R.  D.  Alexander 
&  D.  W.  Tinkle  (Eds.)  Natural  selection  and  social  behavior. 
New  York:  Chiron  Press,  1981. 

Krames,  L.,  Carr,  W.  J.,  &  Bergman,  B.  A  pheromone  associated  with 
social  dominance  among  male  rats.  Psychonomic  Science,  1969,  16, 
11-12. 

Krames,  L.,  Costanzo,  D.  J.,  &  Carr,  W.  J.  Response  of  rats  to  odors 
from  novel  versus  original  sex  partners.  Proceedings  of  the  75th 
Annual  Convention  of_  the  American  Psychological  Association,  1 967 , 
Z,  117-118. 

Krebs,  J.  R. ,  &  Davies,  N.  B.  An  Introduction  to  behavioural  ecology. 
Sunderland,  Massachusetts:  Sinauer  Associates,  Inc.,  1981. 

Kritzman,  E.  B.  Ecological  relationships  of  Peromyscus  mapiculatus 
and  Pp-rognathus  parvus  in  eastern  Washington.   Journal  of.  Mammalogy, 
1974,  55,  172-188. 

Labov,  J.  B.  Factors  influencing  infanticidal  behavior  in  wild  male 
hn.i^p  mir.fi  (Mus  musculus).  Behavioral  Ecology,  and  Sociobiology, 
1980,  6_,  297-303. 

Laessle,  A.  M.  A  report  on  succession  studies  of  selected  plant 
communities  of  the  University  of  Florida  Conservation  Reserve, 
Welaka,  Florida.   Florida  Academy  of  Science,  1958,  21, 
101-112.  (a) 

Laessle,  A.  M.  The  origin  and  successful  relationship  of  sandhill 
vegetation  and  sand-pine  scrub.  Ecological  Monographs., 
1958,  28.,  361-387.  (b) 

Laffoday,  S.  K.   A  study_  of  prenatal  and  postnatal  development  JJL  ±h&. 
oldfield  mouse,  Peromyscus  pol ionotus.   (Doctoral  dissertation, 
University  of  Florida,  1957).   Dissertation  Abstracts,  1957,  12, 
2096.   (Publication  No.  22,230) 

Landauer,  M.  R. ,  Banks,  E.  M. ,  &  Carter,  C.  S.  Sexual  preferences  of 
male  hamsters  (Msgocr icetus  auratus)  for  conspecifics  in  different 
endocrine  conditions.  Hormones  and  Behavior,  1977,  9_,  193-202. 


152 


Landauer,  M.  R.,  Seidenberg,  B.  C. ,  &  Santos,  A.  V.  Hormonal  control 
of  sexual  preferences  in  male  and  female  hamsters.  Paper  presented 
at  the  meeting  of  the  Animal  Behavior  Society,  Seattle,  Washington, 
1978. 


Lande,  R.  The  maintenance  of  genetic  variability  by  mutation  in  a 
polygenic  character  with  linked  loci.  Genetics  Research.  1976, 
23.,   221-235. 

Lanier,  D.  L.,  Estep,  D.  0. ,  &  Dewsbury,  D.  A.  Role  of  prolonged 
copulatory  behavior  in  facilitating  success  in  a  competitive  mating 
situation  in  laboratory  rats.  Journal  of  Comparative  and 
Physiological  Psychology.  1979,  91,   781-792. 

Lawton,  A.  D.,  &  Whitsett,  J.  M.   Inhibition  of  sexual  maturation  by 
a  urinary  pheromone  in  male  prarie  deer  mice.  Hormones  and  Behavior, 
1979,  H,    128-138. 

LeBoeuf,  B.  J.  Male-male  competition  and  reproductive  success  in 
Elephant  Seals.   American  Zoologist.  1974,  1A,    163-176. 

LeBoeuf,  B.  J.,  &  Peterson,  R.  S.   Social  status  and  mating  activity  in 
Elephant  Seals.  Science.  1969,  16J.,  91-93. 

Lidicker,  W.  Z.,  Jr.  The  social  biology  of  the  California  vole.  T_h_e. 
Biologist.  1980.  62,  46-55. 

Linduska,  J.  P.  Winter  rodent  populations  in  field-shocked  corn. 
Journal  pi  Wildlife  Management.  1942,  £,  353-363. 

Llewel lyn,  J.  B.  Seasonal  changes  in  the  aggressive  behavior  of 
Peromyscus  maniculatus  inhabiting  pinyon-juniper  woodlands  in 
western  Nevada.   Journal  of  Mammalogy.  1980,  6_L,  341-345. 


Lombard!,  J.  R.,  &  Whitsett,  J.  M.  Effects  of  urine  from  conspecifics 
on  sexual  maturation  in  female  prairie  deer  mice,  Peromyscus 
maniculatus  bairdi  i.   Journal  of  Mammalogy.  1980,  6_L,  766-768. 


153 


Mainardi,  D.,  Marsan,  M.,  &  Pasquali,  A.  Causation  of  sexual 
preferences  of  the  house  mouse.  The  behavior  of  mice  reared  by 
parents  whose  odor  was  artificially  altered.  Estratto  dagl_j_ 
Atti  del  la  Societa  Ital  iana  dj_  Scienze  Natural  i  e  dej_  Museo  Civico 
dlfStorla  Naturale  di  Ml  jane,  1965,  1Q4,  325-338. 

Mai  lory,  F.  F.,  &  Brooks,  R.  J.   Infanticide  and  other  reproductive 
strategies  in  the  collared  lemming,  Dicrostonx  groen  I  and  icu.s. 
Nature.  1978,  211,  144-146. 

Martell,  A.  M. ,  &  Macaulay,  A.  L.  Food  habits  of  deer  mice  (Perpmyscus 
maniculatus)  in  Northern  Ontario.  Canadian  Field.- Natural  ist, 
1981,  21,  319-324. 

Martin,  R.  E.  Species  preferences  of  allopatric  and  sympatric 
populations  of  silky  pocket  mice,  genus  Perognathus  (Rodentia: 
Heteromyidae).  American  Midland  Naturalist,  1977,  28.,  124-136. 

Maynard  Smith,  J.  Fertility,  mating  behavior,  and  sexual  selection 
in  Drosophi la  subobscura.   Journal  of  Genetics.  1956,  14,  261-279. 

Maynard  Smith,  J.  Evolution  and  the  theory  of  games.  American 
Scientist.  1976,  6_4,  41-45. 

Maynard  Smith,  J.  Parental  investment:  A  prospective  analysis.  Animal 
Behaviour.  1977,  75.,    1-9. 

Maynard  Smith,  J.  The  evolution  of  £3&l  Cambridge:  Cambridge 
University  Press,  1978. 

Maynard  Smith,  J.,  &  Price,  G.  R.  The  logic  of  animal  conflict.  Nature, 
1973,  241,  15-18. 

Mayr,  E.  Populations,  species,  and  evolution.  Cambridge,  MA:  Harvard 
University  Press,  1970. 

McDonald,  D.  L.,  &  Forslund,  L.  G.   The  development  of  social 

preferences  in  the  voles  Mlcrotus  montanus  and  Microtus  canicaudus: 
effects  of  cross-fostering.   Behavioral  BloJflgy.  1978,  22,  497-508. 

McGuire,  M.  R. ,  &  Getz,  L.  L.   Incest  taboo  between  sibling  Microtus 
ochrogaster.   Journal  flf  Mammalogy.  1981,  12,  213-215. 


154 


Merrett,  R.  B.,  &  Wu,  B.  J.  On  the  quantification  of  promiscuity  (or 
promyscus  Maniculatus?)   Evolution.  1975,  29,  575-578. 

Metzgar,  L.  H.  Dispersion  patterns  in  a  Peromyscus  population. 
Journal  of  Mamma iogv.  1979,  60,  129-145. 


Metzgar,  L.  H.  Dispersion  and  numbers  in  Peromyscus  populations. 
American  Midland  Naturalist.  1980,  101,  26-31. 


Mihok,  S.  Behavioral  structure  and  demography  of  subarctic 
Clethrionomvs  gapperi  and  Peromyscus  maniculatus.   Canadian. 
Journal  of  Zoology.  1979,  5J7_,  1520-1535. 

Miller,  W.  C.  Ecological  and  ethological  isolating  mechanisms  between 
Microtus  pennsylvanicus  and  Microtus  ochrogaster  at  Terre  Haute, 
Indiana.   American  Midland  Naturalist.  1969,  82,  140-148. 


Mills,  J.  A.  The  influence  of  age  and  pair-bond  on  the  breeding 
biology  of  the  red-bi I  led  gul I  Larus  novaehol landiae  scapul im 
Journal  of  Animal  Ecology.  1973,  42,  147-163. 


Morris,  R.  F.  Population  studies  on  some  small  forest  mammals  in 
eastern  Canada.   Journal  of  Mammalogy.  1955,  3Ji,  21-35. 


Murphy,  M.  R.   I ntraspeci f ic  sexual  preferences  of  female  hamsters. 
Journal  of  Comparative  and  Physiological  Psychology,  1977,  9J_, 
1337-1346. 


Murphy,  M.  R.  Sexual  preferences  of  male  hamsters:   Importance  of 

preweaning  and  adult  experience,  vaginal  secretion,  and  olfactory  or 
vomeronasal  sensation.  Behavioral  and  Neural  Biology.  1980,  3.0, 
323-340. 


Nakatsuru,  K.,  &  Kramer,  D.  L.   Is  sperm  cheap?  Limited  male  fertility 
and  female  choice  in  the  lemon  tetra  (Pices,  characidea) .   Science, 
1982,  216,  753-754. 


Nisbet,  I.  C.  T.   Courtship-feeding,  egg-size  and  breeding  success  in 
common  terns.   Nature.  1973,  24J_,  141-142. 


0'Donald,  P.   Possibility  of  assortive  mating  in  the  Arctic  skua. 
Nature.  1959,  183,  1210-1211. 


155 


O'Donnell,  Y.,  Blanchard,  R.  J.,  &  Blanchard,  D.  C.  Mouse  aggression 
increases  after  24  hours  of  isolation  or  housing  with  females. 
Behavioral  and  Neural  Biology.  1981,  22,  89-103. 


Orians,  G.  H.  On  the  evolution  of  mating  systems  in  birds  and  mammals. 
American  Natural  ist.  1969,  1Q3_,  589-603. 


Packer,  C.   Inter-troop  transfer  and  inbreeding  avoidance  in  Papio 
anubus.   Animal  Behaviour,  1979,  Z7_,  1-36. 


Parker,  G.  A.  Sexual  selection  and  sexual  conflict.   In  M.  S.  Blum 
N.  A.  Blum  (Eds.)  Sexual  selection  and  reproductive  competition 
in  insects.  New  York:  Academic  Press,  1979. 


Partridge,  L.  Mate  choice  increases  a  component  of  offspring  fitness 
in  fruit  flies.  Nature.  1980,  281,  290-291. 


Poole,  T.  B.,  &  Morgan,  H.  D.  R.   Social  and  territorial  behaviour 
of  laboratory  mice  (Mus  musculus  L.)  in  small  complex  areas.  Animal 
Behaviour,  1976,  21,  476-480. 


Pusey,  A.  E.   Inbreeding  avoidance  In  chimpanzees.  Animal  Behaviour, 
1980,  28.,  543-552. 


Ralls,  K.  Mammals  in  which  females  are  larger  than  males.  Quarterly 
Review  of  Biology.  1976,  51,  245-276. 


Ralls,  K.,  Brugger,  K.,  &  Ballou,  J.   Inbreeding  and  juvenile  mortality 
In  small  populations  of  ungulates.  Science.  1979,  206_,  1101-1103. 


Rand,  A.  L.,  &  Host,  P.  Results  of  the  Archbold  expeditions.  No.  45. 
Mammal  notes  from  Highlands  County,  Florida.  Bulletin  of  the  American 
Museum  of  Natural  History,  1942,  80.,  1-21. 

Randall,  J.  A.  Behavioral  mechanisms  of  habitat  segregation  between 
sympatric  species  of  Microtus:  Habitat  preference  and  interspecific 
dominance.   Behavioral  Ecology  and  Sociobiology,  1978,  3_,  187-202. 


Rasmussen,  D.  I.  Blood  group  polymorphism  and  inbreeding  In  natural 
populations  of  the  deer  mouse  Peromyscus  maniculatus.  Evolution, 
1964,  18.,  219-229. 


156 


Rasmussen,  D.  I.  Biochemical  polymorphisms  and  genetic  structure  in 
populations  of  Peromyscus.  Symposium  of  the  Zoological  Society  of 
London,  1970,  26.,  335-349. 


Rathbun,  G.  B.  The  social  structure  and  ecology  of  elephant-shrews. 
Zeitschrift  fur  Tierpsycholoqy,  Advances  in  ethology  supplement,  1979, 
22.,  1-80. 


Redfield,  J.  A.,  Krebs,  C.  J.,  &  Taitt,  M.  J.  Competition  between 
Peromyscus  maniculatus  and  Microtus  townsendi  i  in  grasslands  of 
coastal  British  Columbia.  Journal  of  Animal  Ecology.  1977,  4_6_, 
607-616. 


Re i mar,  J.  D.,  &  Petras,  M.  L.  Breeding  structure  of  the  house  mouse, 
Mus  musculus.  in  a  population  cage.  Journal  of  Mammalogy.  1967, 
4£,  88-99. 


Rice,  L.  Z.  A  study  of  food  hoarding  in  freely  growing  laboratory 
populations  of  prairie  deermice.   Unpublished  masters  thesis,  College 
of  Will iam  and  Mary,  1972. 

Ruddy,  L.  L.  Discrimination  among  colony  mates'  anogenital  odors  by 
guinea  pigs  (Cavia  porcel lus) .  Journal  of  Comparative  and 
Physiological  Psychology,  1980,  21,  767-774. 

Sadleir,  R.  M.  F.  S.  The  relationship  between  agonistic  behavior  and 
population  changes  in  the  deermouse  Peromyscus  maniculatus  (Wagner). 
Journal  of  Animal  Ecology.  1965,  21,  331-352. 

Schwagmeyer,  P.  L.  The  Bruce  effect:   An  evaluation  of  male/female 
advantages.  American  Natural ist.  1979,  111,  932-938. 


Scott,  J.  P.  Agonistic  behavior  of  mice  and  rats:  A  review. 
American  Zoologist,  1966,  6_,  683-701. 


Scott,  J.  P.,  &  Frederickson,  E.  The  causes  of  fighting  in  mice  and 
rats.   Physiological  Zoology.  1951,  21,  273-309. 


Selander,  R.  K.  Behavior  and  genetic  variation  in  natural  populations. 
American  Zoologist.  1970,  1Q_,  53-66. 


157 


Selander,  R.  K.,  Smith,  M.  H.,  Suh,  Y.  Y.,  Johnson,  W.  E.  &  Gentry,  J. 
B.   IV.  Biochemical  Polymorphism  and  systematics  in  the  genus 
Peromyscus.   I.  Variation  in  the  old-field  mouse.  Studies  in  genetics 
VI.  University  of  Texas  Publication  7103,  1971. 

Seligman,  M.  E.  P.  On  the  generality  of  the  laws  of  learning. 
Psychological  Rev i ew .  1970,  22,  406-418. 

Shields,  W.  M.   Inbreeding  and  the  paradox  of  sex:  A  resolution? 
Evolutionary  Theory.  1982,  1,   245-279. 

Siege  I,  S.  Nonparametric  statistics  for  the  behavioral  sciences. 
New  York:  McGraw-Hill  Book  Company,  Inc.,  1956. 

Simon,  N.  G.  The  genetics  of  intermale  aggressive  behavior  in  mice: 
Recent  research  and  alternate  strategies.  Neurosqience  & 
Biobehavloral  Reviews.  1979,  3,  97-106. 

Smith,  M.  H.  The  evolutionary  significance  of  certain  behavioral, 
physiological ,  and  morphological  adaptations  of  the  old-field 
mouse.  Peromyscus  pol ionotus   (Doctoral  dissertation, 
University  of  Florida,  1966).   Dissertation  Abstracts.  1967, 
21,   4692B-4693B.   (University  Microfilms  No.  67-372) 

Smith,  M.  H.  Mating  behavior  of  Peromyscus  pol ionotus.  Quarterly 
Journal  of  the  Florida  Academy  of  Sciences.  1967,  3J1,  230-240. 

Smith,  M.  H.  Dispersal  of  the  old-field  mouse,  Peromyscus  pol Ionotus. 
Bulletin  of  the  Georgia  Academy  of  Science,  1968,  26.,  45-51. 

Smith,  M.  H.  Food  as  a  limiting  factor  in  the  population  ecology  of 
Peromyscus  pol  ionotus.  Annales  Zoologici  Fennici,  1  97 1 ,  8_, 
109-112. 

Smith,  M.  H.,  &  Blessing,  R.  W.  Trap  response  and  food  availability. 
Journal  of  Mammalogy.  1969,  50.,   368-369. 

Smith,  M.  H.,  Carmon,  J.  L.,  &  Gentry,  J.  B.  Pelage  color  polymorphism 
in  Peromyscus  pol  ionotus.   JourjiaJ  _oJ _Mamma I ogy ,  1972,  5J.,  824-833. 


158 


Smith,  M.  H.,  &  Criss,  W.  E.  Effects  of  social  behavior,  sex  and 

ambient  temperature  on  the  endogenous  diel  body  temperature  cycle  of 
the  old-field  mouse,  Peromyscus  pol ionotus.  Physiological  Zoology, 
1967,  60,  31-39. 

Smith,  M.  H.,  Garten,  C.  T. ,  Jr.,  &  Ramsey,  P.  R.  Genie  heterozygosity 
and  population  dynamics  in  small  mammals.   In  C.  L.  Markert  (Ed.) 
I sozymes  IV:  Genetics  and  Evolution.  New  York:  Academic  Press,  1975. 

Smith,  M.  H.,  &  McGinnis,  J.  T.  Relationships  of  latitude,  altitude,  and 
body  size  to  litter  size  and  mean  annual  production  of  offspring 
in  Peromyscus.  Researches  on  Population  Ecology.  1968,  ]£, 
115-126. 


Stacey,  P.  B.  Female  promiscuity  and  male  reproductive  success  in 
social  birds  and  mammals.  American  Natural  ist.  1982,  120.,  51-64, 


Stickel,  L.  F.  Home  range  and  travels.   In  J.  A.  King  (Ed.)  Biology  of 
Peromyscus  (Rodentia) .  Lawrence,  Kansas:   American  Society  of 
Mammalogi sts,  1968. 

Sugiyama,  Y.  On  the  social  change  of  Hanuman  langurs  (Presbytis 

entel I  us)  in  their  natural  conditions.  Primates,  1965,  6,  381-418. 


Taitt,  M.  J.  The  effect  of  extra  food  on  small  rodent  populations:  I. 
deermice  (Peromyscus  manicu I atus) .  Journal  of  Animal  Ecology.  1981, 
50,  111-124. 

Terman,  C.  R.  Some  dynamics  of  spatial  distribution  within  semi-natural 
populations  of  prairie  deermice.  Ecology.  1961,  4£,  288-302. 

Terman,  C.  R.  Population  dynamics.   In  J.  A.  King  (Ed.)  Biology  of 
Peromyscus  (Rodentia) .   Lawrence,  Kansas:   American  Society 
of  Mammalogi sts,  1968. 

Terman,  C.  R.   Laboratory  Studies  of  Population  Regulation  in  Prairie 
Deermice.   Centennial  Symposium  on  Science  and  Research,  1974, 
Philadelphia  Zoo,  Philadelphia  PA. 

Terman,  C.  R.   Presence  of  the  female  and  aggressive  behavior  in  male 
prairie  deermice  (Peromyscus  maniculatus  bairdi) .  Paper  presented 
at  meetings  of  the  Animal  Behavior  Society,  Duluth,  Minnesota, 
August,  1982. 


159 


Thiessen,  D.  D.,  &  Maxwell,  K.  0.  A  glass  rodent  enclosure:  Gerbil 
city.  Behavior  Research  Methods  &  Instrumentation,  1979,  JUL,  535-537. 

Thomas,  J.  A.,  &  Birney,  E.  C.  Parental  care  and  mating  system  of 
the  prarie  vole,  Microtus  ochrogaster.  Behavioral  Ecology  and 
Sociobiology,  1979,  5_,  171-186.  -------- 

Thorpe,  W.  H.  Bird  song.  London:  Cambridge  University  Press,  1961. 

Tinbergen,  N.  K.  Th^  ^"dy  of  instinct.  Oxford:  Oxford  University 
Press,  1951 . 

Trivers,  R.  L.  Parental  investment  and  sexual  selection.   In  B. 
Campbell  (Ed.)  Sexual  selection  and  the  descent  of  man,  1871-1971. 
Chicago:   Aldine,  1972. 

Trivers,  R.  L.  Sexual  selection  aid  resource-accruing  abilities  in 
Anol  is  garmani.  EvolutionP  1976,  3_0_,  253-269. 

Vaughan,  T.  A.   Mammalogy.  Philadelphia:   W.  B.  Saunders  Company,  1978. 

Verner,  J.  Evolution  of  polygamy  in  the  long-billed  marsh  wren. 
Fvolution.  1964,  18.,  252-261. 

Verner,  J.,  &  Willson,  M.  F.  The  influence  of  habitats  on  mating  systems 
of  North  American  passerine  birds.  Ecology.  1966,  41,  143-147. 

Vestal,  B.  M.,  &  Hellack,  J.  J.  Comparison  of  neighbor  recognition  in  two 
species  of  deer  mice  (Peromyscus) .  Journal  of  Mammalogy,  1978, 
5_9_,  339-346. 

Wade,  M.  J.  Sexual  selection  and  variance  in  reproductive  success. 
American  Naturalist.  1979,  J_L4,  742-746. 

Ward,  S.  E. ,  Baumgardner,  D.  J.,  &  Dewsbury,  D.  A.  Experimental 
determinants  of  female  mating  preference  in  Microtus  ochrogaster 
and  NL  montanus.  Paper  presented  at  meetings  of  the  Animal  Behavior 
Society,  Knoxville,  Tennessee,  June,  1981. 


160 


Weatherhead,  P.  J.,  &  Robertson,  R.  J.  Offspring  quality  and  the 

polygyny  threshold:  "the  sexy  son  hypothesis!*   American  Natural ist. 
1979,  111,  201-208. 


Webster,  D.  G. ,  Sawrey,  D.  K.,  Williams,  D.  C. ,  &  Dewsbury,  D.  A. 
Apparatus  for  assessment  of  rodent  social  preference.  Paper 
presented  at  the  meetings  of  the  Animal  Behavior  Society,  Duluth, 
Minnesota,  1982. 


Webster,  D.  G. ,  Williams,  M.  H.,  &  Dewsbury,  D.  A.  Female  regulation 
and  choice  in  the  copulatory  behavior  of  Montane  voles  (Microtus 
montanus) .  Journal  of  Comparative  and  Physiological  Psychology, 
1982,  26,  661-667. 

Webster,  D.  G. ,  Williams,  M.  H.,  Owens,  R.  D.,  Geiger,  V.  B.,  & 

Dewsbury,  D.  A.  Digging  behavior  in  twelve  taxa  of  muroid  rodents. 
Animal  Learning  &  Behavior.  1981,  9_,  173-177. 

Wecker,  S.  C.  The  role  of  early  experience  in  habitat  selection  by  the 
prairie  deer  mouse,  Peromyscus  maniculatus  bairdil.  Ecological 
Monographs.  1963,  31,  307-325. 

Whitney,  C.  L.,  &  Krebs,  J.  R.  Mate  selection  in  Pacific  tree  frogs. 
Nature.  1975,  211,  325-326.  (a) 


Whitney,  C.  L.,  &  Krebs,  J.  R.  Spacing  and  calling  in  Pacific  tree 
frogs,  Hyla  regi I  la.  Canadian  Journal  of  Zoology.  1975,  51,  1519- 
1527.  (b) 


Whitsett,  J.  M.,  Gray,  L.  E.,  Jr.,  &  Bediz,  G.  M.  Gonadal  hormones  and 
aggression  toward  juvenile  conspecifics  in  prairie  deermice. 
Behavioral  Ecology  and  Sociob iology.  1979,  6,  165-168. 


Wickler,  W. ,  &  Seibt,  U.  Monogamy  in  Crustacea  and  Man.  Zeitschrift 
fur  Tierpsvchology.  1981,  H,  215-234. 

Williams,  G.  C.  Adaptation  and  natural  selection:   A  critique  of  some 
current  evolutionary  thought.  Princeton,  N.J.:   Princeton  University 
Press,  1966. 


Williams,  0.  Food  habits  of  the  deer-mouse.   Journal  of  Mammalogy, 
1955,  4£,  415-420. 


161 


Williams,  R.  G.,  Golley,  F.  B.,  £  Carmon,  J.  L.   Reproductive 
performance  of  a  laboratory  colony  of  P_«.  po|  ionotus.  American 
Mjrilanri  Natural ist.  1965,  73,  101-110. 

Wilson,  E.  0.   Sociobioloay:  The  New  Synthesis.  Cambridge:  Harvard 
University  Press,  1975. 

Wilson,  J.  R. ,  Kuehn,  R.  E.,  &  Beach,  F.  A.  Modification  in  the  sexual 
behavior  of  male  rats  produced  by  changing  the  stimulus  female. 
Journal  of  Comparative  and  Physiological  Psychology,  1 963,  56., 
636-644. 


Wittenberger,  J.  F.  The  evolution  of  mating  systems  in  birds  and 

mammals.   In  P.  Marier  &  J.  Vandenberg  (Eds.)  Handbook  of  behavioral 
neurobiology.  Vol.  3.  Social  behavior  and  communication,  New  York: 
Plenum,  1979. 

Wittenberger,  J.  F.  Animal  social  behavior.  Boston:   Duxbury  Press, 
1981. 


Wittenberger,  J.  F.,  &  Tilson,  R.  L.  The  evolution  of  monogamy: 
Hypotheses  and  evidence.   Annual  Review  of  Ecology  and.  Systemics, 
1980,  11,  197-232. 

Zahavi,  A.   Nate  selection — A  selection  for  a  handicap.   Journal  of 
Theoretical  Biology.  1975,  53,  205-214. 

Zucker,  I.,  £  Wade,  G.   Sexual  preferences  of  male  rats.   Journal  of 
Comparative  and  Physiological  Psychology.  1968,  66_,  816-819. 


BIOGRAPHICAL  SKETCH 

I  was  a  member  of  the  "baby  boom,"  born  to  George  E.  and 
Mary  L.  Webster  on  May  5,  1948.   I  started  my  life  in  Oshkosh, 
Wisconsin.   Three  moves  later  the  family  was  settled  in  my  parents 
present  home  in  Rothschild,  Wisconsin.   I  completed  high  school  in 
neighboring  Schofield  at  D.  C.  Everest  (home  of  the  "Evergreens")  in 
1966,  and  continued  my  education  for  another  two  years  at  the  Marathon 
Campus  of  the  University  of  Wisconsin,  in  Wausau,  Wisconsin.  Toward 
the  end  of  1968  I  met  Carole  ...  and  followed  her  south  to  New 
Orleans,  where  we  were  married  in  July  of  1969.   In  August  of  1969  I 
became  a  member  of  the  U.  S.  Air  Force  and  served  four  years  as  an 
oral  surgery  technician  at  Keesler  A.F.B.,  Biloxi,  Mississippi.  Our 
daughter,  Danielle,  was  born  in  1972  just  prior  to  our  leaving  the 
service  and  rejoining  the  student  population.   I  completed  the 
requirements  for  my  B.S.  at  the  University  of  Wisconsin  in  Madison, 
Wisconsin,  in  1976,  and  we  headed  back  south  to  the  University  of 
Florida  where  I  completed  the  requirements  for  the  M.S.  in  1979. 


162 


I  certify  that  I  have  read  this  study  and  that  In  my 
opinion  It  conforms  to  acceptable  standards  of  scholarly 
presentation  and  is  fully  adequate,  In  scope  and  quality, 
as  a  dissertation  for  the  degree  of  Doctor  of  Philosophy. 


Dr.  Donald  A.  Dewsbury,  Chairman 
Professor  of  Psychology 


I  certify  that  I  have  read  this  study  and  that  In  my 
opinion  it  conforms  to  acceptable  standards  of  scholarly 
presentation  and  is  fully  adequate,  in  scope  and  quality, 
as  a  dissertation  fcr  the  degree  of  Doctor  of  Philosophy. 


/ 


kfAdiim. 


Dr.  Merle  E.  Meyer 
Professor  of  Psychology 


I  certify  that  I  have  read  this  study  and  that  In  my 
opinion  It  conforms  to  acceptable  standards  of  scholarly 
presentation  and  is  fully  adequate,  In  scope  and  quality, 
as  a  dissertation  for  the  degree  of  Doctor  of  Philosophy. 


A/ 

y 


'/  ■>,,yj,av^/- 


Dr.  Wi Ise  B.  Webb 

Graduate  Research  Professor  of  Psychology 


I  certify  that  I  have  read  this  study  and  that  in  my 
opinion  it  conforms  to  acceptable  standards  of  scholarly 
presentation  and  is  fully  adequate,  in  scope  and  quality, 
as  a  dissertation  for  the  degree  of  Doctor  of  Philosophy. 

Dr.  Carol  Van  Hartesveldt 
Associate  Professor  of  Psychology 


I  certify  that  I  have  read  this  study  and  that  in  my 
opinion  it  conforms  to  acceptable  standards  of  scholarly 
presentation  and  is  fully  adequate,  in  scope  and  quality, 
as  a  dissertation  for  the  degree  of  Doctor  of  Philosophy. 


;C^    (1 


Dr.  H.  Jane  Brockmann 
Associate  Professor  of  Zoology 


This  dissertation  was  submitted  to  the  Graduate  Faculty  of 
the  Department  of  Psychology  in  the  College  of  Liberal  Arts 
and  Sciences  and  to  the  Graduate  Council,  and  was  accepted 
as  partial  fulfillment  of  the  requirements  for  the  degree 
of  Doctor  of  Philosophy. 


April  1983  Dean  for  Graduate  Studies 

and  Research 


UNIVERSITY  OF  FLORIDA 


3  1262  08553  5945