Skip to main content

Full text of "Analysis of audit judgment through an expert system"

See other formats


FACULTY  WORKING 
PAPER  NO.  982         ocTHB 


DEC.  5 


3>S 


Analysis  of  Audit  Judgment  Through 
an  Expert  System 

Christopher  Dungan 
John  S.  Chandler 


College  of  Commerce  and  Business  Administration 
Bureau  of  Economic  and  Business  Research 
University  of  Illinois,  Urbana-Charnpaign 


BEBR 


FACULTY  WORKING  PAPER  NO.  982 
College  of  Commerce  and  Business  Administration 
University  of  Illinois  at  Urbana-Champaign 
November  1983 


Analysis  of  Audit  Judgment  Through  an  Expert  System 


Christopher  Dungan 
University  of  South  Florida  (Sarasota) 

John  S.  Chandler,  Assistant  Professor 
Department  of  Accountancy 


This  research  was  supported  by  a  grant  from  the  Peat, 
Marwick,  Mitchell  &  Co.,  Research  Opportunities  in  Auditing 
Program. 

This  is  a  working  paper  and  should  not  be  quoted  or  reproduced 
in  whole  or  in  part  without  the  written  consent  of  the  authors 
Comments  and  suggestions  should  be  forwarded  to  the  authors. 


Digitized  by  the  Internet  Archive 

in  2011  with  funding  from 

University  of  Illinois  Urbana-Champaign 


http://www.archive.org/details/analysisofauditj982dung 


Abstract 


The  goal  of  the  exploratory  research  described  in  this  paper  was 
to  create  an  interactive  computer  program  which  would  function  as  a 
dynamic,  operating  model  of  auditors'  judgment  and  provide  a  tool  for 
investigating  the  domain  of  audit  judgment.   The  system  would  be  operated 
in  a  noisy,  natural  setting  to  make  credible  audit  decisions.   The 
auditor's  evaluation  of  the  adequacy  of  the  Allowance  for  Bad  Debts  con- 
stituted the  functional  area  of  interest  in  this  study.   The  result  of 
this  work  was  the  creation  of  AUDITOR — a  model  of  an  audit  judgment  in 
the  form  of  an  interactive  computer  program  called  an  expert  system. 
This  paper  describes  how  AUDITOR  was  designed  and  constructed.   The 
results  of  two  successful  validation  exercises  are  presented.   A  discus- 
sion of  the  insights  into  audit  judgment  provided  by  AUDITOR  concludes 
this  paper. 


INTRODUCTION 

Interest  in  the  methods  by  which  accountants  make  professional 
judgments  has  stimulated  considerable  research  in  recent  years.   Com- 
prehensive reviews  of  this  work  may  be  found  in  Ashton  (1982)  and  Libby 
(1981).   Whether  effected  via  regression  modeling  or  focused  on  the 
probabilistic  aspects  of  judgment,  it  is  generally  agreed  that  some  of 
the  research  to  date  incorporates  in  its  design,  features  which  may 
operate  as  limitations  on  the  external  validity  of  the  work.   That  is, 
typically,  a  relatively  passive  subject  has  been  requested  to  perform 
a  highly-structured  and  well-defined  task  via  a  simplified  response 
scale  in  a  laboratory  setting  while  utilizing  a  limited  set  of  data 
with  which  he  has  been  provided. 

Several  approaches  have  been  used  to  mitigate  these  limitations, 
for  example,  by  introducing  less-restricted  search  behavior.   Biggs  and 
Mock  (1980)  avoided  the  introduction  of  any  restriction  on  subjects' 
cue  choices  by  studying  their  spontaneous  verbal  protocols  as  they  per- 
formed in  an  experimental  setting.   Abdel-Khalik  and  El-Sheshai  (1980) 
allowed  subjects  to  expand  the  number  of  factors  which  might  be  examined 
by  choosing  their  own  cues  from  among  those  provided  in  a  shopping  list. 
Shields  (1980)  allowed  subjects  to  make  choices  of  cues  from  information 
boards  in  a  study  of  the  effect  of  information  load  upon  information 
search  patterns. 

Like  the  work  on  verbal  protocols,  this  present  study  utilizes 
self-reports  from  the  subjects  themselves  instead  of  inferring  a  deci- 
sion model  from  the  relationships  between  outputs  and  inputs.   Although 
the  validity  of  self-reports  of  mental  processes  is  the  subject  of  on- 
going debate,  c.f.  Ericsson  and  Simon  (1980),  Nisbett  and  Wilson  (1977), 
Payne,  Braunstein,  and  Carroll  (1978),  Einhorn,  Kleinmuntz,  and  Kleinmuntz 
(1979),  certain  aspects  of  the  present  study  distinguish  it  from  previous 
work. 

The  goal  of  this  present,  exploratory  work  was  to  create  an  inter- 
active computer  program  which  would  function  as  a  dynamic,  operating 
model  of  auditor's  judgment.   The  system  would  be  operated  in  a  noisy, 
natural  setting  to  make  creditable  audit  decisions.   While  operating 
within  the  confines  of  the  available  programming  technology,  the  expert 
auditors  who  participated  in  building  the  system,  themselves,  freely 
determined  the  cues,  their  weights,  and  the  form  and  size  of  the  system. 
The  result  of  this  work  was  the  creation  of  AUDITOR — a  model  of  an  audit 
judgment  in  the  guise  of  an  interactive  computer  program  of  a  type  pro- 
perly identified  as  an  expert  system. 

Auditors'  evaluation  of  the  adequacy  of  the  Allowance  for  Bad 
Debts  (ABD)  constituted  the  functional  area  of  interest  in  this  study. 
This  particular  audit  judgment  was  chosen  for  study  and  modeling  for 
several  reasons.   It  was  one  of  the  judgments  most  frequently  named  by 
the  experts  themselves  when  asked,  "What  critical  audit  judgment 
requires  the  attention  of  an  experienced  professional  and  is  never 
entrusted  to  'green'  auditors?" 


-2- 


Also,  when  auditors  make  judgments  of  the  adequacy  of  the  ABD  they 
are  presented  at  a  subsequent  visit  to  the  client's  office  with  an  oppor- 
tunity for  a  referent  outcome — the  "outcome  feedback"  which  is  sometimes 
considered  to  be  a  necessary  condition  to  sharpen  and  calibrate  the 
expert's  skills  (Harrell,  1977;  Ashton,  1981).   That  is,  at  a  later 
date  auditors  can  observe  from  the  client's  records  which  accounts 
have  indeed  proved  to  be  uncollectible.   Such  a  judgment  seemed  to  the 
researchers  more  likely  to  provoke  the  consensus  which  Einhorn  (1974) 
felt  to  be  necessary  requisite  for  expertise.   Thus,  in  the  absence  of 
any  other  procedure  for  assuring  that  the  decision  process  chosen  for 
study  would  be  one  which  would  clearly  be  recognized  as  demonstrating 
expert  judgment,  the  researchers  believed  expert  judgment  in  auditing 
would  most  likely  be  fostered  where  the  judgments  were  made  under  con- 
ditions which  provided  the  judges  with  the  possibility  of  objective 
outcome  feedback.   Finally,  this  area  of  -judgment  gave  promise  of  trac- 
tability  in  initial  interviews  with  the  experts,  in  that  they  seemed 
able  to  recite  the  cues  to  which  they  attend  when  making  the  judgment, 
and  the  cues  seemed  similar  among  several  experts. 


EXPERT  SYSTEMS 

Expert  systems  are  computer  programs  which  offer  consultative 
advice  in  a  bounded  knowledge  domain  on  a  level  of  competence  often 
rivaling  that  of  a  human  practitioner  who  is  recognized  as  an  expert  in 
his  field.   Such  systems  are  constructed  by  a  system-builder  utilizing 
the  active  collaboration  of  one  or  more  of  the  experts  themselves,  who 
are  challenged  to  justify  the  manner  in  which  they  make  their  iudgments 
These  systems  are  now  providing  valuable  consultative  advice  to  clients 
in  real-world  settings  (Michie,  1980). 

Expert  systems  trace  their  lineage  to  attempts  in  the  1950s  to 
create  computerized  problem-solving  routines  of  broad  generality,  for 
example  the  Logic  Theorist  program  of  Newell,  Simon,  and  Shaw  (1958). 
This  program  and  later  generalized  problem-solvers  such  as  ARGUS 
(Reitman,  1965)  and  GPS  (Ernst  and  Newell,  1969)  made  contributions 
to  subsequent  work  but  did  not  themselves  display  great  breadth  of 
achievement.   More  recent  work  proceeds  from  a  belief  that  the  high- 
level  skill  demonstrated  by  a  human  expert  derives  from  his  accumulated 
experience  of  a  concentrated  nature  which  enables  him  to  perform  opti- 
mally in  situations  to  which  that  experience  is  pertinent  but  is  of 
little  help  in  genuinely  novel  situations  (Simon,  1978). 

Although  frequently  applied  to  programs  of  medical  diagnosis  or 
treatment,  expert  systems'  technology  appears  to  be  appropriate  wher- 
ever evidence  of  less  than  certain  reliability  must  be  evaluated  by  a 
skilled  practitioner  according  to  experientially  based  rules  in  order 
to  make  a  judgment.   Thus,  the  field  of  auditing  bears  similarities 
to  domains  already  approached  via  the  technology  of  expert  systems. 
Comments  of  workers  in  the  field  of  "knowledge  engineering,"  as  it  is 
sometimes  called,  support  this  analogy. 


-3- 


The  domain  is  one  in  which  diverse  factors  must  be 
identified  and  synthesized  to  form  judgments, 
evaluate  alternatives,  and  make  decisions.   Years 
of  experience  are  brought  to  the  problem  at  hand; 
experience  and  subjective  judgment  play  a  major 
role.   The  domain  is  not  easily  amenable  to  precise 
scientific  formulation.   (Duda  et  al . ,  1979) 

The  domain  lacks  a  strong  mathematical  structure,  is 
incorrigibly  non-numerical,  and  is  too  complex  for 
adequate  analytical  specification.   (Michaelson,  1982) 

The  knowledge  which  the  expert  brings  to  the  task  is 
largely  heuristic  knowledge,  experimental,  uncertain — 
mostly  good  guesses  and  good  practice  in  lieu  of 
facts  and  rigor — much  of  this  private  to  the  expert. 
How  else  explain  internships  of  guild-like  apprentice- 
ship to  a  presumed  master  of  the  craft?   What  the 
master  really  knows  is  not  written  in  the  textbooks. 
(Feigenbaum,  1979). 

Examples  of  successful,  currently  operating  expert  systems  are 
frequently  reported  in  the  popular  press  as  instances  of  "artificial 
intelligence."   For  example,  MYCIN  infers  disease  identity  in  blood  or 
meningitis  infections  and  suggests  antibiotic  treatment  (Shortliffe  and 
Buchanan,  1975).   PROSPECTOR  evaluates  core  samples  to  infer  the  pre- 
sence of  significant  mineral  deposits  (Duda  et  al.  1979).   AL/X  diagnoses 
causes  of  shutdowns  occurring  under  the  control  of  automatic  safety 
devices  on  oil  production  rigs  (Reiter,  1980). 

AL/X  (as  did  MYCIN)  included  in  its  design  features  the  goal  of 
separation  of  domain-specific  knowledge  from  the  computer  control  pro- 
gram which  utilizes  the  knowledge.   Thus,  while  AL/X  was  originally 
expert  in  only  one  realm  of  application  its  inference  structure  provides 
a  foundation  upon  which  can  be  built  expert  systems  in  other  domains. 
AUDITOR  utilizes  the  inference  structure  and  control  capabilities  of 
AL/X,  described  in  more  detail  in  Appendix  [A] .   For  a  fuller  treatment 
of  its  capabilities  see  Dungan  (1983)." 


AUDITOR  FROM  A  USER'S  PERSPECTIVE 


The  AUDITOR  model  was  constructed  in  three  distinct  phases:   initial 
modeling,  refinement,  and  validation,  all  of  which  will  be  described  in 
the  course  of  this  paper.   However,  certain  aspects  of  this  process  of 
system-building  can  best  be  understood  after  the  system  has  been  de- 
scribed in  use. 

When  a  user  operates  the  system  interactively  at  a  computer  terminal 
for  purposes  of  consulting  AUDITOR  for  its  advice  concerning  the  large, 
delinquent  receivable  (which  is  the  object  of  this  inquiry),  the 


-4- 


system  asks  him  a  series  of  questions  about  the  extent  of  his  personal 
knowledge  of  the  presence  or  absence  of  the  evidence  cues  required  in 
the  rules.   AUDITOR  begins  each  of  its  queries  with  the  phrase,  "How 
certain  are  you..."?  which  is  prefaced  to  each  of  the  rules  in  the 
rule  base.   For  example,  one  query  will  be  worded,  "How  certain  are 
you  that  recent  collections  toward  the  delinquent  portions  of  this 
account  are  proceeding  satisfactorily?"   The  user's  response  provides 
a  means  for  the  system  to  acquire  data  about  the  underlying  cue, 
suggested  by  the  experts,  which  in  this  case  concerns  the  debtor's 
recent  payment  performance. 

The  user  responds  to  the  system's  query  by  typing  a  numeral  called 
a  certainty  value  (CV).   CVs  represent  subjective  certainty  (or  uncer- 
tainty) on  a  scale  from  -5  to  5.   The  response,  "5,"  is  given  by  the 
user  of  the  system  when  he  is  certain  that  the  matter  referred  to  in 
the  query  is  true.   When  he  is  certain  the  matter  is  false  his  response 
is  "-5."    In  effect,  the  CV  scale  from  .1  to  5.0  in  absolute  terms 
(unsigned)  communicates  increasing  amounts  of  certainty,  to  which  a 
positive  sign  is  attached  for  truth  and  a  minus  sign  for  falsity.  When 
the  user  has  no  information  about  the  matter,  he  is  unable  to  judge  its 
truth  or  falsity,  or  he  feels  the  question  is  irrelevant,  he  responds 
with  "0."   The  researchers  did  not  investigate  individual  differences 
in  the  use  of  this  scale,  although  the  process  appears  somewhat  akin  to 
the  extraction  of  a  utility  curve,  c.f.  Newton  (1977). 

AUDITOR  utilizes  the  data  in  the  user's  response  in  two  ways. 
First,  it  updates  the  current  strength  of  belief  of  the  hypothesis, 
which  in  AUDITOR  is  stated  in  degrees  of  belief  (DB),  (DB  =  10*logl0  (Odds)) 
The  nature  of  the  updating  depends  upon  the  nature  of  the  links  between 
the  rules  and  the  hypothesis:   Bayesian  (for  IF: THEN  links),  Logical 
(for  AND,  OR,  NOT),  or  contextual  (adaptable  for  unique  linkages). 

Second,  the  system  inspects  its  rule  base  to  determine  which  one  of 
the  remaining  questions  to  ask  next.   It  does  this  by  following  criteria 
built  into  the  AL/X  control  (unless  they  have  been  modified  by  contex- 
tual links).   The  criteria  are  simply  stated:   Ask  that  question  next 
which  could  possibly  impact  greatest  upon  the  DB  of  the  hypothesis ,  con- 
sidering the  inventory  of  questions,  their  diagnostic  links,  the  current 
DB  of  the  hypothesis,  and  the  CV  responses  which  might  be  given  by  the 
user.   Thus,  AUDITOR'S  inquiry  proceeds  in  economical  fashion  down  that 
line  of  questioning  which  could  have  the  maximum  impact  upon  the  hypo- 
thesis . 

This  process  of  query,  response,  and  update  continues  until  (1)  all 
questions  in  the  system  have  been  exhausted,  (2)  the  user  terminates 
the  session,  or  (3)  the  updating  process  has  caused  the  DBs  of  the  hypo- 
thesis to  reach  a  threshhold  level  beyond  which  further  questioning 
could  cause  no  significant  improvement,  according  to  criteria  adjustable 
within  the  system.   In  any  of  these  cases,  the  system  can  then  report 
its  conclusion,  which  constitutes  its  "expert  judgment"  in  the  form  of 
a  statement  of  the  current  degrees  of  belief  to  which  the  hypothesis  has 
been  updated.   In  a  typical  consultation  with  AUDITOR  this  report  might 
read: 


-5- 


The  delinquent  portion  of  this  account  should  speci- 
fically be  reserved-for  in  the  allowance  for  bad 
debts  to  a  substantial  extent:   RESERVE.   Prior  degree 
was  0.0,  current  degree  is  -19.5.   At  this  point  this 
goal  is  probability  .05  or  less. 

This  report  informs  the  user  of  the  system  that  the  data  provided 
by  him  has  changed  the  degree  of  belief  of  the  hypothesis  from  its  ini- 
tial state,  0.0  (.5  probability),  to  -19.5  (approximately  .013  proba- 
bility).  At  this  point  the  user  must  decide  for  himself  whether  or  not 
-19.5  DB  (.013  probability)  dictates  to  him  a  course  of  action,  since 
there  is  insufficient  experience  with  AUDITOR  to  provide  guidance  to 
relate  these  DB  and  probabilities  definitively  to  decision  points  in  an 
expert's  judgment  process. 


CONSTRUCTION  OF  THE  SYSTEM 

A  model  of  expert  judgment,  constructed  in  the  form  of  an  expert 
system,  is  assembled  from  components  consisting  of: 

One  or  more  hypotheses  stating  the  judgment  which  is 
the  end  product  of  the  expert's  reasoning  process. 

Rules  which  express  the  relationship  between  the 
evidence  (cues)  and  the  hypothesis. 

Parameters  which  express  (1)  the  diagnostic  value 
or  impact  which  the  expert  believes  are  implied  by 
each  cue,  and  (2)  the  strength  of  the  expert's 
beliefs  prior  to  examination  of  any  evidence,  that 
is,  the  initial  state  of  the  system. 

These  components  were  assembled  and  tested  during  three  stages: 
initial  modeling,  refinement,  and  validation.   Initial  modeling  encom- 
passed  development  of  the  hypothesis  which  is  the  goal  of  the  system, 
interviews  with  the  auditing  experts  to  elicit  cues,  conversion  of 
these  cues  into  rules,  and  polling  of  the  experts  to  elicit  parameters 
adequate  to  construct  a  preliminary,  yet  operating,  system.   In  the 
refinement  stage,  experts  operated  the  model  interactively  on  a  com- 
puter terminal  and  presented  their  suggestions  for  improvements  to  be 
integrated  into  the  system.   These  improvements  consisted  primarily  of 
changes  in  parameters,  additions  of  interactions  between  the  rules,  and 
a  few  instances  of  rewording  of  the  cues.   Finally,  the  completed  sys- 
tem was  exposed  to  validation  by  testing  it  to  learn  the  extent  to  which 
a  different  group  of  experts  would  similarly  acknowledge  it  to  be  expert 
in  its  performance.   (Further  references  within  this  paper  to  "experts" 
will  mean  the  several  auditors  who  participated  in  the  several  stages 
of  building,  refining,  and  validating  the  AUDITOR  system.) 


-6- 


INITIAL  MODELING:   HYPOTHESIS 

Expert  systems  in  other  knowledge  domains  are  constructed  under  a 
belief  that  an  expert's  information  search  and  processing  are  focused 
upon  and  organized  around  hypotheses,  one  or  more  of  which  ultimately 
will  express  the  judgment  which  he  will  render  upon  the  data.   There 
is  speculation  that  the  predecisional  behavior  of  accountants  and  audi- 
tors is  similarly  organized  around  hypotheses  (Libby,  1981).   After 
selection  of  the  judgment  area  for  modeling  the  next  step  in  the  con- 
struction of  AUDITOR  consisted  of  formulating  an  hypotheses  to  reflect 
the  expert's  judgment.   In  an  expert  system  this  judgment  is  referred 
to  as  the  goal  hypothesis. 

AUDITOR'S  single,  goal  hypothesis  is  expressed,  "The  delinquent 
oortion  of  this  account  should  specifically  be  reserved  for  in  the 
allowance  for  bad  debts  to  a  substantial  extent."   (For  ease  of  refer- 
ence and  recognition,  the  name  of  this  hypothesis,  RESERVE,  as  well 
as  the  name  of  each  rule  in  the  system  will  be  written  in  capital 
letters.)   The  phrase,  "this  account"  refers  to  the  one  individually 
large  account — or  in  some  cases  a  single  invoice — which  is  under  scru- 
tiny by  the  auditor.   "To  a  substantial  extent"  was  agreed  among  the 
participants  of  this  project  to  mean  all  cases  in  which  they  judged 
recovery  is  likely  to  be  no  more  than  an  amount  considered  insignifi- 
cant in  relation  to  the  delinquent  balance. 

RESERVE  is  intended  to  reflect  the  approach  which  the  experts 
testified  they  visualized  when  scrutinizing  the  individually  large, 
delinquent  accounts  of  a  commercial,  audit  client.   It  can  be  labelled 
a  "worst-yet-possible-case"  approach.   This  approach  envisions  that 
each,  individually  large  account  (or  invoice)  be  judged  collectible  or 
uncollectible.   The  magnitude  of  the  client's  allowance  for  bad  debts 
(ABD)  is  then  considered  adequate  or  inadequate  in  comparison  to  the 
aggregate  of, the  large,  delinquent  accounts  which  have  been  judged  un- 
collectible. 


DEVELOPMENT  OF  THE  RULE  BASE 

The  rule  base  for  AUDITOR  was  developed  from  the  cues  provided  by 
four  practicing  auditors  chosen  from  among  the  eight  who  were  consulted 
initially  when  choosing  an  area  for  modeling.   Those  actively  partici- 
pating in  the  bulk  of  the  project  were  three  managers  and  one  senior 
on  the  audit  staff  of  a  CPA  firm  of  international  scope.    (By  compari- 
son, for  the  development  of  expert  systems  in  other  fields  the  whole- 
hearted cooperation  of  one  practitioner  who  is  clearly  expert  is  often 
considered  adequate.) 

In  individual  interviews  these  auditing  experts  recalled  and  named 
the  cues  to  which  they  said  they  attend  when  evaluating  the  collecti- 
bility of  a  client's  delinquent  receivables.   For  example,  one  cue 


-7- 


relates  to  whether  the  debtor  continues  to  be  an  active  customer.   Other 
cues  relate  to  the  opinions  of  the  client's  credit  manager  as  to  collec- 
tibility of  the  delinquent  account,  response  to  confirmation  requests, 
and  contents  of  the  client's  credit  file  on  the  delinquent  customer. 
A  total  of  twenty-five  such  cues  were  obtained,  differing  widely  among 
themselves  in  the  diagnostic  impact  which  they  have  upon  the  auditor's 
conclusion.   The  complete  list  may  be  examined  in  Dungan  (1983). 

After  eliminating  from  the  list  obvious  redundancies  and  a  few 
errors  in  transcribing,  the  researchers  then  transformed  each  of  the 
cues  into  a  simple  rule  in  IF:THEN  form.   For  example,  a  cue  named  by 
one  of  the  experts  was:   "The  customer's  stated  intent  regarding  pay- 
ment."  This  became  the  rule  called  NOTPAY:   "The  customer  has  stated 
his  intent  to  pay  little  or  nothing  of  the  delinquent  account,"  which 
is  processed  by  AUDITOR  utilizing  a  Bayesian  revision  as  IF, NOTPAY:  THEN 
(to  the  extent  determined  by  parameters),  RESERVE.   The  connector  AND 
was  later  added  (during  the  refinement  stage  of  the  work)  in  order  to 
provide  linkages  between  rules  said  by  the  experts  to  be  interrelated. 
The  connector  NOT  was  used  occasionally  to  achieve  a  more  natural  word- 
ing of  a  rule.   (Although  available  for  use  through  AL/X,  the  connector 
OR  was  not  used. ) 


PARAMETERS 

Next,  parameters  were  developed  for  the  rules — the  diagnostic 
weights  which  express  what  the  experts  believe  to  be  the  evidential 
importance  of  each  of  the  cues.   This  step  began  with  a  polling  of  the 
four  auditing  experts.   Each  of  their  rankings  of  "strong,"  "moderate," 
"weak,"  and  "no  effect"  for  each  of  the  rules  were  translated  into  a 
quantitative  scale  on  an  expedient  and  preliminary  basis.   For  example, 
all  four  experts  rated  the  impact  of  NOTPAY  as  "strong."   These  pre- 
liminary values  were  later  changed  to  the  values  contained  in  the  final 
version  of  AUDITOR  when  the  experts  themselves  operated  the  system  and 
offered  criticisms  aiding  in  its  refinement. 

AL/X  expresses  the  diagnostic  impact  of  the  evidence  in  the  form  of 
Positive  Weights  (PW)  and  Negative  Weights  (NW).   As  the  AUDITOR  system 
is  operated  interactively,  the  degree  of  belief  attached  to  the  goal 
hypothesis  (RESERVE)  is  incremented  by  a  value  which  is  a  function  of 
the  PW_  and  the  Certainty  Value  (CV)  of  the  user's  response  if  the  user 
has  keyed-in  a  positive  CV  in  answer  to  the  system's  query,  and  upon  the 
NW  and  the  CV  when  the  user's  response  is  a  CV  in  the  negative  range. 
The  role  of  PW  and  NW  are  explained  more  fully  in  Appendix  [A] . 


REFINEMENT 

Armed  with  a  basic  set  of  rules  and  parameters  the  researchers 
assembled  a  working  model  of  AUDITOR  with  the  aid  of  the  AL/X  software 
package.   For  purposes  of  refinement,  this  working  model  was  returned 
to  the  experts  to  secure  their  suggestions  for  improvements.   As  a 


-8- 


result  of  this  refinement  stage,  major  changes  were  made  in  the  PW  and 
NW,  numerous  interactions  were  added,  and  a  few  changes  in  the  composi- 
tion of  the  rule  base  were  found  to  be  desirable.   An  example  of  these 
changes,  the  rule  called  WORKOUT  was  originally  developed  from  a  cue 
which  expressed  the  auditor's  interest  in  a  delinquent  debtor's  perfor- 
mance toward  fulfilling  a  "workout  alreement"  or  similar  negotiated 
understanding  between  the  client  and  the  debtor.   Before  refinement  the 
rule  was  stated,  "Payments  are  being  received  currently  under  a  workout 
agreement."   As  a  result  of  reactions  received  during  refinement  of  the 
system,  this  rule  was  restated  to  read,  "Recent  collections  toward  the 
delinquent  portions  of  this  account  are  proceeding  satisfactorily,"  in 
order  to  eliminate  the  restrictive  nature  of  the  reference  to  "workout 
agreements . " 

Also  at  this  stage  of  the  work  interactions  in  the  form  of  AND 
statements  were  added  to  the  rule  base  upon  the  suggestions  of  the 
experts.   For  example,  the  individual  effects  of  the  rules  called  LEGAL 
and  NOTPAY  are  enhanced  when  both  of  these  cues  are  found  present  at 
the  same  time.   That  is,  when  a  delinquent  debtor  has  stated  his  intent 
not  to  pay  (NOTPAY)  and  the  same  debtor  also  presents  a  counterclaim 
which  would  appear  to  make  legal  action  fruitless  (LEGAL),  the  AUDITOR 
system  operates  by  implementing  each  of  these  rules  individually  and 
also  implements  a  third  rule,  LEGAL&NPAY,  which  carries  its  own  impact 
(in  the  form  of  PW  and  NW)  upon  the  degree  of  belief  in  the  goal  hypo- 
thesis . 

After  interactions  had  been  inserted,  to  accomplish  further  refine- 
ment the  experts  operated  the  system  in  "full  trace"  mode  (so  called). 
Although  this  mode  was  slow  in  operation,  even  tedious,  it  allowed  the 
experts  to  observe  the  effect  upon  the  hypothesis  which  might  be  caused 
by  alternative  possible  responses  of  a  user,  thus  stimulating  them  to 
suggest  changes  in  the  values  of  the  parameters,  PW  and  NW.   The  goal 
sought  by  these  changes  was  to  calibrate  AUDITOR  to  the  extent  that  a 
probability  of  at  least  90%,  (DB  9.5)  should  be  reported  by  AUDITOR  at 
the  time  of  the  expert's  own  report  of  a  subjective  feeling  of  "satis- 
faction" in  his  judgment,  despite  the  difficulties  with  this  proce- 
dure which  might  be  predicted  by  a  review  of  recent  research,  c.f. 
Lichtenstein,  Fischoff,  and  Phillips  (1977),  Crosby  (1981),  and  others, 
summarized  both  in  Libby  (1981)  and  in  Ashton  (1982).   Although  consensus 
was  found  among  the  experts  in  the  direction  of  the  changes  which  they 
suggested,  the  model  proved  somewhat  insensitive  to  the  exact  values  to 
which  che  changes  were  made,  a  result  reminiscent  of  the  work  of  Dawes 
(1975)  and  of  Einhorn,  Kleinmuntz,  and  Kleinmuntz  (1979),  a  matter  which 
will  be  explored  in  a  subsequent  paper.   The  complete  set  of  rules  and 
parameters  utilized  in  the  system  may  be  examined  in  Appendix  [C]. 


VALIDATION 

The  results  of  AUDITOR'S  operations  were  compared  against  the 
judgment  of  practicing  auditors  utilizing  client  audit  data  contained 
in  work  papers.   Two  validation  procedures  were  employed:   "Open-book" 
and  "Blind." 


-9- 


OPEN  BOOK:   The  validation  procedure  referred  to  as  "Open-book"  was 
performed  by  an  audit  manager  in  a  different  office  of  the  same  CPA  firm 
which  participated  in  the  building  of  the  system.   This  auditor,  serving 
as  validator,  selected  work  papers  covering  completed  audits  of  two  com- 
mercial clients  with  which  he  personally  was  not  involved.   From  the  work 
papers  of  each  of  the  two  clients  the  validator  chose  five  individually 
large,  delinquent  accounts.   The  criteria  which  he  used  for  his  selec- 
tion was  not  communicated  to  the  researchers.   Each  set  of  work  papers 
to  which  he  referred  contained  the  decisions  made  by  the  auditors  during 
the  audit  about  their  assessment  of  the  collectibility  of  these  five 
delinquent  accounts  (and  others).   Since  the  validator  was  unfamiliar 
with  the  two  clients  which  he  had  selected,  the  work  papers  presumably 
also  contained  all  the  information  from  which  he  determined  his  responses 
to  AUDITOR'S  questions.    Since  to  the  validator  it  was  obvious  which 
set  of  judgments  appeared  in  the  work  papers  and  which  "judgments"  were 
produced  by  AUDITOR,  this  procedure  is  referred  to  as  an  open-book 
validation. 

After  a  demonstration  of  operation  of  the  system  by  the  researchers, 
the  validator  invoked  the  AUDITOR  program  and  responded  to  its  ques- 
tioning by  entering  information  which  he  gathered  from  the  work  papers. 
That  is,  he  responded  to  its  inquiries  by  entering  a  Certainty  Value  on 
a  scale  from  -5  to  5  in  answer  to  each  question  presented  to  him  follow- 
ing the  prefix,  "How  certain  are  you...?" 

In  the  case  of  each  delinquent  account,  the  validator  himself 
decided  when  to  exit  from  the  system,  thus  concluding  one  session  with 
AUDITOR.   Presumably,  that  occurred  either  when  he  believed  that  he  had 
provided  to  AUDITOR  enough  data  by  which  he  personally  could  have  made 
a  decision  concerning  collectibility  of  each  account,  or  when  all  data 
available  in  the  work  papers  had  been  entered  into  the  system. 

At  the  conclusion  of  each  session  the  validator  relayed  to  the  re- 
searcher the  judgment  of  the  CPAs  who  had  performed  the  audit.   He  also 
examined  AUDITOR'S  report  of  degrees  of  belief  and  probabilities  which 
had  been  produced  as  a  consequence  of  the  interactive  question  and  answer 
session.   On  the  basis  of  his  own  criteria  he  labeled  each  result  a  "hit" 
(appropriate  result)  or  a  "miss."   (In  every  case  he  commented  that  his 
own  judgment  regarding  the  write-off  of  the  delinquent  account  agreed 
with  that  reported  in  the  work  papers.)   The  results  are  summarized  in 
Table  1. 

RESULTS:   In  each  case  but  one,  when  the  validator  chose  to  ter- 
minate questioning  and  call  for  the  report  by  the  system,  AUDITOR 
reported  a  degree  of  belief  which  was  the  equivalent  of  a  probability 
of  at  least  .865  in  favor  of  the  same  decision  as  that  called  for  by 
the  original  audit  team — that  is,  either  in  favor  of,  or  against,  the 
need  for  an  allowance  for  the  delinquent  account  under  scrutiny.   The 
validator  and  the  researchers  considered  these  results  to  be  successful. 

BLIND  VALIDATION:   A  second  validation  procedure  was  carried  out  to 
conform  to  the  suggestions  made  by  the  mathematician  A.  M.  Turing  (1950). 


a 

•-4 

CC 

re 

03 

— 

c 

L 

E 

c 

iH 

0) 

L 

c 

<2_ 

•H 

•rJ 

73 

« 

cu 

co 

4) 

g 

•1-1 

4-> 

M 

< 

o 

Li 

u 

o_ 

0) 

o 

c 

u 

c 

c 

0) 

2 

OS 

c 

H 

1)    C 


< 


z 
c 
I— I 
E- 
< 


< 

> 


C/3 


Cu 


C/2 

— 

a 
co 
Ed 
os 


si 
< 


co 


3   <     3 

L 

>>  03 
<4-i  J2  4J 
O  -H 

73 

•   cu  73 

o  .*    3 

z    o    re 

> 

c 


>, 

oi 

-C 

CO 

73 

r-"» 

o; 

E 

2 

u 

0) 

i-i 

— 

jj 

c 

CO 

03 

D« 

>. 

c 

01 

CO 

•r^ 

:*: 

CC 

a: 

be 

GO 

^-v 

< 

01 

H 

jj  o  u 

•H    3     > 

--i  <  a! 


03 

u 

a. 


0) 

c 

> 

c 

u 

o 

Ol 

CU 

4) 

« 

> 

> 

03 

m 

o> 

u 

L 

OJ 

i— ) 

> 

0) 

0) 

od 

<o- 

Li 

03 

03 

0) 

0> 

a 

o 

li 

03 

PA 

os 

c 

> 

0) 

o 

L 

o3 

c 

c 

•> 

0) 

z 

z 

en 

03 

0) 

a! 

sC 


CJ> 
C7* 


I 

a 

X 

O 

c 
c 


c 

CM 

O 

■H 

73 

- 

•«     0! 

•H 

4-1      4-1 

c    o 

L 

««"«» 

0>      0! 

CJ 

— • 

3   <— I 

03 

—1 

a-  i-i 

01 

•H 

c    o 

e 

s: 

•i-     o 

CO 


0)    >, 

T3    >H 

4J 

c    c 

O     0) 


f^     CU 


o 
o 
o 

<r 
<o- 

u_ 

C 


OJ 

cr 
c 
•H 

a>   c 

73    >H 

3 
C     C 

o 

o   ^ 

•  CCJ 
X  J-i 
r^    o 

CO-    4-1 


m 


03 
0 

o 

bC 

o 
c  -a 

C     0) 

•  tc 
in    re 

■H     E 

<j>   re 


j->  L 

c  o 

3  n-i 

o 

CJ  E 

CJ  -H 

re  re 

l-H  CJ 

re 


0) 

C  3 
O  CT 
C    c 


in    cy 
co-  -c 


0) 

> 

u 
o 

03 

01 

06 


CN 


in 

oo 


i->. 

av 

vD 

CTn 

CTN 

& 

o 

CT> 

01 

J3 

CJ 
Li 

re 

01 

CO 

cu 
J-I 


•a 

cu 

73 
L 

o 
u 

0> 

u 


0) 

73 


co 

CC 


t-i 

0) 

CJ 

u 
re 
cu 

03 

01 

- 


-c 

Ui 

c 
o 
cu 

L 


o> 

T3 


0 
Z 


c 
re 


c 

CJ 

E 
C 
•H 

3 
cr 

re 

0) 


01 

t: 

0) 

bC 

Ui 

re 


c 

01 
CJ 

X 

0) 

Ol 
4-i    bO 

3     Li 

o>    re 

U   £. 
L     CJ 


CN 


*  < 

4-1 

c 

CU 


0) 

> 

Li 

01 
03 
Ol 

OS 

o 
z 


Oi 

L 

01 
03 
01 
OS 


CU 

> 

u 

01 
03 

0) 

OS 

o 

z 


0) 

> 

Li 

01 
03 

01 


CN 


in 


o> 


in 
cc 


en 
cr 


en 


cu 

CJ 

c 
re 

re 

J3 


C 
01 

3 

cr 

01 
CO 

X3 
3 
03 

XI 
CU 

u 

cj    oi 

0>    -u 

^^    re 
c 

CJ     J-l 

0) 

i— I    cu 


03 
03 
0) 

c 

•H 
03 

3 
J3 


cu 


<      03  O 


en 


cr 

c 


01 

c 
c 
c 

<o- 


c 

3 
L 

C 

re 

03 


Ol 
Q 


m 


u  X 

C  I  • 

0>  Li  U  *C 

03  re  —•  cu 

Oi  C  3  £ 

Li  03  CJ 

o.  co  cu  re 

CU  -H  L  CU 

L  x.  U 

re;  -h  jj 


3 
O 
3 

C 

c 


os   re 
o 

H     3 

Q    -H 
33  -a 

<     -H 
> 

c   o 

■H     L 

a 


.c   c 

ju     CU 

E 

u-    be 

O  TD 
3 

Li    '— 

O 

>    u 

re  j3 


C      03 


CU 
03 


3  w-< 

cu  n 

i—i  CU 

re  cu 

o  c 

03 

cu 

_  _ 


CU 

i-i  03 

i— I  CU 

0)  03 

> 

0  re 

03  E 

cu  cu 

CU  4-1 

U  03 

&C  >^ 

CU  03 
TJ 

:  as 
O 

OJ  E-i 

—  — 

33 

E  < 
O 

U  01 

U-l  £ 

4-1 

T3 

CU  03 

4_l  C 

1  re 
cu  cu 
>  E 

c 

C  4-1 

CJ  03 

c 

03  t- 

o)  re 

._i  tc 

4-1  < 

-J  C» 

■H  C3> 

X  • 

re 

£}  •> 

O  03 

L  3 

a  j= 

E- 

cu 

s_ 

re  • 

03  4J 

i-i  c 

re  i-i 

E  re 


L 
&-S    o 

cr  o. 
o^    cu 

L 

a 

3  : 

C 
03  C 
^  -H 
CJ      CO 

re  1-4 

4-1  O 
03     CU 

CJ 

B     03 

0)  - 

4-1  E 

03  U 

>,  -H 

CO  Cu 

0>  < 
J3  CU 
4-1    C_3 


C    T3 

01 

L    73 

oi   re 

CO    cu 

3   J3 

01     3 

J3    E 

4-J     3 

i— ( 

>>  c 
x    a 

73     01 
Ol    j3 

X   E- 

03 
•H 

3      • 
L     >> 

3     !-i 

u-i   re 

CO 

CO     CO 

re   cu 

CJ 

»  cu 

CU     3 
CJ 


X 


■•  c 

4J     3 

c 

3     4-1  • 

o   re  co 

CJ    J3  L 

CJ    4-i  0 

re  4J 

•>  i-i 

L     03  73 

re  tj  3 

—i  co 


CJ 


cj    u    cj   re   oi 

o    a  vi  ^  i 

~        CJ      4-1     4-1     4J 


-11- 


This  procedure  is  considered  to  be  a  more  stringent  test  of  the  validity 
of  an  expert  system  and  has  become  somewhat  of  a  standard  in  the  field 
(c.f.  Yu,  1979).   Turing,  when  confronted  with  the  issue  of  whether  com- 
puters and  their  programs  could  be  said  to  think,  proposed  instead  that 
the  testable  question  is  whether  an  observer  who  is  himself  ignorant — 
that  is,  blind — as  to  the  identity  of  the  source  could  distinguish 
between  the  output  of  the  machine  and  that  of  a  human. 

Blind  validation  of  AUDITOR  utilized  two  human  auditors,  one  who 
served  as  user  of  the  system,  the  other  as  validator.   Both  auditors 
were  managers  with  an  international  CPA  firm  not  previously  involved  in 
the  AUDITOR  project.   The  auditor  who  performed  as  user  selected  an 
audit  client  with  which  he  was  familiar.   His  choice  was  a  NYSE  listed 
company  which  manufactures  and  markets  through  various  distribution 
channels  consumer  products  such  as  cigarettes  and  candies.   His  audit 
team,  in  their  visit  to  the  client's  offices  at  an  interim  date,  had 
identified  by  computerized  selection  eleven  invoices  so  large  and 
delinquent  as  to  justify  the  auditors'  individualized  attention.   The 
criteria  for  this  selection  was  not  communicated  to  the  researchers. 

The  user  was  familiarized  with  the  AUDITOR  system  and  then  in- 
structed to  respond  to  its  questions  using  the  data  contained  in  the 
work  papers.   Since  he  himself  had  been  a  member  of  the  audit  team  it 
is  not  possible  to  determine  if  his  answers  were  based  solely  upon  the 
contents  of  the  work  papers  or  perhaps  also  relied  upon  his  memory  for 
information  not  in  the  papers.   He  answered  the  system's  questions, 
responding  on  the  CV  scale  from  -5  to  5.   As  in  the  first  validation, 
the  user  himself  decided  when  to  terminate  each  session.   Concurrently, 
he  reported  relevant  data  from  the  work  papers  to  the  researcher  who 
recorded  it,  such  as  age,  dollar  balance,  confirmation  results,  nature 
of  customer's  business,  etc.   This  data — recorded  on  Fact  Sheets — became 
part  of  the  raw  data  presented  to  the  validator  from  which  he  made  his 
judgment.   The  user  also  reported  the  audit  teams'  judgment,  just  as  it 
was  recorded  in  the  work  papers.   This  information  was  recorded  by  the 
researchers  on  a  Comparison  Worksheet,  on  which  was  also  entered  the 
"judgment"  of  the  AUDITOR  system. 

Subsequently,  the  audit  manager  serving  as  validator  was  presented 
with  the  Comparison  Worksheets  which  contained  the  judgments  from  the 
two  sources  and  the  Fact  Sheets  containing  the  data  from  which  they 
made  their  judgments.   (Of  course,  the  sources  of  each  judgment  were 
disguised  when  presented  to  the  validator;  tbat  is,  he  was  "blind"  as 
to  the  source — auditor  or  AUDITOR — of  each  judgment.)   The  task  which 
he  undertook  was  to  study  the  same  evidence  on  the  Fact  Sheets  which 
had  been  available  to  the  Fact  Sheets  which  had  been  available  to  the 
two  experts,  auditor  and  AUDITOR,  to  form  his  own  independent  and  pre- 
sumably expert  conclusion,  and  then  to  accept  or  reject  each  judgment 
from  each  source  on  the  basis  of  his  opinion  of  the  expertise  which  each 
had  demonstrated.   That  is,  he  accepted  or  rejected  each  of  twenty-two 
recommendations  which  were  disguised  as  to  source. 


-12- 


RESULTS:   The  validator  accepted  all  of  the  audit  team's  judgments 
and  all  but  one  of  AUDITOR'S.   These  results  are  summarized  in  Tables 
2  and  3.   In  sum,  over  the  two  validation  procedures,  open-book  and 
blind,  the  AUDITOR  system  scored  nineteen  hits  out  of  twenty-one  cases. 


ANALYSIS 

TAXONOMY 

In  AUDITOR  a  high  score  on  one  cue  can  offset  a  low  score  on  a 
different  cue.   Thus,  AUDITOR  can  be  called  a  compensatory,  rather  than 
non-compensatory  model.   For  example,  credit  to  the  delinquent  customer 
may  not  have  been  stopped  by  the  client  (CREDITSTOP) ,  and  the  customer 
may  have  demonstrated  a  good  record  in  the  past  of  paying  his  account 
(GOODRECORD) .   However,  both  of  these  cues,  favorable  to  an  expectation 
of  collectibility  of  the  account,  may  be  offset — compensated  for — by 
evidence  that  the  customer  is  in  bankruptcy  proceedings  (BANKRUPTCY) 
and  the  client's  lawyer  advises  that  recovery  is  unlikely  (LAWYER). 

AUDITOR  incorporates  probabilistic  aspects  of  decision  making,  as 
do  other  successful  expert  systems,  recognizing  that  no  evidence  is 
perfectly  diagnostic  of  the  condition  which  the  expert  must  evaluate 
and  that  real-world  judgments  are  made  on  the  basis  of  information 
which  must  be  probabilistically  evaluated.   The  query  format,  "How  cer- 
tain are  you...?"  reminds  the  system's  user  to  perform  a  subjective 
assessment  of  the  reliability  of  the  evidence. 

In  contrast  with  Lens  Model  studies,  and  indeed  with  most  studies 
of  auditors'  judgments  except  those  utilizing  Verbal  Protocol  Analysis, 
AUDITOR  processes  the  independent  variables  with  which  it  operates,  in 
the  form  of  user's  CV  applied  to  the  PW  or  NW  of  a  cue,  in  a  sequential 
fashion.   That  is,  AUDITOR  continuously  revises  and  expands  the  system 
as  its  queries  are  successively  put  to  and  answered  by  the  user.   In 
sum,  AUDITOR  incorporates  subjective  assessment  of  the  evidence  through 
what  is  basically  a  Bayesian  revision  process  operating  on  cues  which 
can  compensate  for  each  other.   Therefore,  the  AUDITOR  system  can  be 
called  a  subjective,  Bayesian,  compensatory  expert  system,  which  func- 
tions as  a  sequential  model  of  auditors'  judgment. 


COMPARISON  WITH  OTHER  STUDIES 

Two  previous  studies  have  investigated  auditors'  perceptions  of 
what  was  called  source  reliability  (Joyce  and  Biddle,  1980)  or  source 
credibility  (Bamber,  1980,  quoted  in  Libby,  1981).   Joyce  and  Biddle 
explored  whether  auditors  weighted  differently  the  reliability  of  infor- 
mation depending  upon  its  origin  with  the  client's  credit  manager  or 
with  an  outside  credit  reporting  agency.   Joyce  and  Biddle  concluded 
that  unless  both  sources  were  called  to  the  auditors'  attention  (as  was 
done  in  a  within-subjects  experiment)  they  did  not  differently  weight 
the  sources.   In  the  AUDITOR  project  no  rule  incorporating  outside  credit 
reports  was  suggested  by  the  experts,  and  apparently  none  was  referenced 


-13- 


TABLE  2 


CONVERSION  OF  DEGREE  AND  PROBABILITY  INTO  VERBAL  JUDGMENTS 


AUDITOR'S  Reported 

AUDITOR  was    Degree  of  Belief  AUDITOR'S  Result 

Concealed  as     and  Equivalent  Was  Reported  to 

Case  #   Expert  No»       Probability  Validator  as 


"OK,  no  res.  nor  adj." 

"No  reserve  nor  adj . 
needed" 

"Yes ,  reserve  or  adj . 
req  'd" 

"No" 

"No" 

"Yes" 


DB 

% 

3-1 

II 

-15.5 

3.2 

3-2 

I 

-19.5 

1.3 

3-3 

II 

15 

97.0 

3-4 

I 

-30 

.01 

3-5 

II 

-17 

2.0 

3-6 

II 

26 

99.0 

3-7 

II 

2 

61.0* 

3-8 

I 

-30 

.01 

3-9 

II 

-30 

.01 

3-10 

I 

-30 

.01 

3-11 

I 

33 

99.9 

'At  most  a  partial  res 
or  adj .  req 'd" 

"No" 

"No" 

"No" 

"Yes" 


*There  were  no  clear  guides  for  interpretation  of  the  implications  of 
AUDITOR'S  result  at  this  level  of  likelihood  (61%  probability  of  need 
for  an  allowance).   Since  the  first  validation  procedure  could  be 
interpreted  as  suggesting  that  partial  allowances  by  auditors  might 
be  associated  with  the  AUDITOR'S  results  at  this  level  of  probability, 
the  researcher  chose  to  state  the  expert's  opinion  as  "At  most  a  par- 
tial res.  (reserve)  or  adj.  (adjustment)  (is)  req'd  (required)". 


-14- 


TABLE  3 


SUMMARY  RESULTS  BLIND  VALIDATION 


Case 
#3-    Brief  Description 

1  $19,292  over  90  da. 
active  gov't  account,  not 
confirmed. 

2  $17,553  over  90  da. 
active  gov't  account,  not 
confirmed . 

3  $16,346,  30-60  da.,  100% 
credit  memo  not  posted.   Not 
confirmed. 

4  $35,909,  30-60  da.,  not 
confirmed.   Workout  agreement 
in  file. 

5  $59,570,  30-60  da.,  not 
confirmed.   Bankrupt  but  letter 
of  credit  in  file. 

6  $13,563,  30-60  da.,  not 
confirmed.   Customer  request 
to  return  goods,  CM.  not 
posted. 

$11,628,  over  90  da., 
confirmed  newer  items  no  excep- 
tion.  Creditmanager  calls 
excellent  account,  but  dispute 
over  price. 

8  $12,498,  30-60  da.,  not 
confirmed,  later  collected. 

9  $14,655,  over  90  da.  not 
confirmed,  later  collected. 

10  $22,024,  over  90  da.  not 
confirmed,  later  collected. 

11  $36,763  delinq.  and  total. 
Letter  in  file:   "Financial 
difficulty,  no  pay  next  six 
months  ." 


Judgments 

AUDITORS    Auditor 

Validator 
Called 

No  reserve 

Accept 

No  res. 

A 

No  res. 

A 

No  res. 

A 

Re  s .  or 
adjust 

Reserve 

A 

A 

No  res. 

" 

A 

No  res  . 

A 

No  re  s . 

A 

No  res. 

A 

Res  .  or 
adjust 

Res .  or 

adjust 

A 

A 

At  most, 
partial 

Reject 

reserve 
or  adj  . 

No  res. 

A 

No  res . 

No  res. 

A 

A 

No  res. 

No  re  s . 

A 

A 

No  res. 

No  res. 

A 

A 

Yes,  res. 


:es,  res 


-15- 


in  the  work  papers — which  covered  large,  commercial  clients — consulted 
by  the  auditors  during  validation  of  the  system.  Therefore,  no  direct 
comparisons  with  Joyce  and  Biddle  are  possible. 

However,  several  other  outside  data  sources  were  included  in  rules 
suggested  by  the  experts  involved  in  building  AUDITOR.   Among  these 
are  PROBLEMS,  relating  to  the  results  from  confirmation  requests, 
LAWYER,  concerning  the  opinion  of  outside  legal  counsel,  NOTPAY,  which 
reports  the  debtor's  stated  intent  not  to  pay  (when  that  is  known), 
and  NONCONTACT,  which  is  a  cue  activated  by  the  client's  and  the  audi- 
tors' inability  to  contact  the  debtor.   These  cues  carry  four  of  the 
five  heaviest,  basic  weightings  (PW  or  NW)  in  AUDITOR.   It  seems 
reasonable  to  believe  that  these  relative  diagnostic  weightings  carry 
within  them  a  factor  which  is  dependent  upon  the  experts'  belief  in 
the  reliability  of  the  source  of  the  information.   Thus,  these  heavier 
relative  weightings  evidently  reflect  the  experts'  belief  in  the 
higher  reliability  of  these  outside  data  sources. 

Additionally,  the  low  diagnostic  weight  accorded  to  the  rule 
CREDITMGR,  which  reports  the  opinion  of  the  client's  credit  manager, 
seems  to  indicate  that  such  a  source,  although  frequently  resorted  to, 
carries  low  reliability.   Bamber's  1980  study  attempted  to  determine 
if  audit  managers  differentially  weighted  the  results  of  work  done  by 
audit  seniors  who  were  described  in  the  experiment  as  being  of  dif- 
fering reliability.   No  comparison  with  the  AUDITOR  project  is 
possible,  since,  as  might  be  expected,  no  cues  suggested  by  the 
experts  focused  on  the  competence  of  the  audit  staff. 


RELATIVE  IMPORTANCE  OF  THE  RULES 

Cues  frequently  used  which  also  have  heavy  diagnostic  weight  are, 
by  implication,  the  major  diagnostic  tools  of  the  experts.   Table  4 
lists  the  cues  in  decreasing  order  of  their  importance  in  use  as  diag- 
nostic indicators  during  the  validation  trials.   This  table  has  been 
prepared,  first,  by  totaling  for  each  cue  the  largest  of  its  Positive 
Weight  or  Negative  Weight  together  with  the  weights  of  the  interactions 
involving  that  cue  (all  in  unsigned  terms).   Then,  this  combined  weight 
has  been  multiplied  by  the  number  of  uses  occurring  during  the  valida- 
tion trials.   (Cues  with  zero  usage  during  validation — implying  that 
no  evidence  concerning  the  cue  was  present  in  the  working  papers — are 
excluded  from  this  table.)   The  result  provides  a  measure  of  the  extent 
to  which  the  system  utilizes  the  different  cues. 

Some  comments  are  appropriate  concerning  the  frequency  of  usage. 
The  figure  for  number  of  uses  is  a  count  of  the  number  of  times  during 
validation  in  which  a  non-zero  Certainty  Value  constituted  the  response 
by  the  system's  user.   Since  twenty-one  delinquent  accounts  were  exa- 
mined during  the  validation  runs,  that  number  is  the  maximum  which  could 
appear  as  number  of  uses.   The  cue  called  COLLECTED  serves  less  as  a 
diagnostic  tool  for  the  experts  than  as  a  means  within  the  AUDITOR 
system  to  identify  which  accounts  continue  to  be  doubtful,  following 


-16- 

TABLE  4 

CUES,  THEIR  WEIGHTS  AND  NUMBER  OF  USES,  AS  DIAGNOSTIC  INDICATORS 

Combined 

Largest    Weight  No.  of  Combined 

Unsigned   W/Inter-  Non-Zero  Weight  X 

Cue                                 PW  or  NW   Actions  Uses  Uses 


COLLECTED  (Account  is  no  longer 
delinquent  by  audit  comple- 
tion date)  30 

ECONOMIC  (Economic  factors  hamper 

ability  to  pay)  3 

BANKRUPT  (Debtor  has  entered  bank- 
ruptcy proceedings)  3 

CORRESPOND  (Recent  data  in  credit 

file  supports  collectibility)  4 

ACTIVE  (Debtor  continues  as  active 

customer)  3 

WORKOUT  (Recent  collections  are 

proceeding  satisfactorily)  3 

NOTPAY  (Debtor  expresses  his  intent 

not  to  pay)  5 

PROBLEMS  (Confirmations  reveals 

serious  problems)  8 

CREDITMGR  (Client  representative 
expresses  strong  belief  in 
collectibility)  2 

LAWYER  (Lawyer  expects  poor  prospects 

of  recovery)  5 

LEGAL  (Debtor  has  strong  counterclaim)      3 

WRITEOFF  (Writeoff  of  this  account 

represents  a  material  adjustment)       2 

COLAGENCY  (Collection  has  been 

assigned  to  agency  or  lawyer)  2 

G00DREC0RD  (Debtor  has  good  record 

of  paying  in  past)  2 

NOPAYEVER  (Debtor  has  made  no  pay- 
ments on  any  invoice)  2 

NONCONTACT  (Confirmation  request 

was  returned  undeliverable)  4.5 


30 


19 


19 


11 


15 


20 


14 


11 


21 
9 

11 


10 


9.5 


13 


13 


600 

171 

152 

143 

105 
78 
70 
55 

45 

42 
36 

22 

21 

20 

20 

9.5 


-17- 


their  initial  identification  and  listing  as  delinquent,  perhaps  during 
an  early  stage  of  the  audit. 

Rules  with  the  highest  usage  reflect  aspects  of  the  auditors' 
apparent  decision  processes  and  usage  of  evidence.   WORKOUT  reflects 
the  significance  to  the  auditor  of  his  knowledge  of  recent  cash  collec- 
tions from  the  delinquent  debtor.   The  phrase,  "proceeding  satisfac- 
torily," in  effect  asks  the  auditor  to  evaluate  the  recency,  regularity, 
and  adequacy  of  the  collections  in  comparison  to  what  he  judges  is  some 
acceptable  standard.   Because  of  the  crucial  nature  of  the  sales  and 
collection  cycle,  the  inspection  and  testing  of  subsidiary  accounts 
receivable  ledgers  and  cash  receipts  records  normally  would  be  part  of 
every  audit.   Thus,  the  information  required  in  WORKOUT  ordinarily  would 
be  available  in  every  audit. 

The  frequent  usage  of  CORRESPOND  similarly  reflects  typical  audit 
practice — that  of  examining  whatever  correspondence  is  available  con- 
cerning a  delinquent  debtor.   To  some  extent,  also,  this  cue  consti- 
tutes a  catch-all.   The  great  variety  of  different  data  which  might  be 
included  in  correspondence  files  by  different  clients  and  for  different 
debtors  discouraged  the  researchers  from  attempting  to  incorporate  in 
AUDITOR'S  rule  base  each  separate  cue  which  might  be  discovered.   For 
example,  one  cue  might  indicate  that  a  delinquent  debtor  has  placed 
with  the  client  a  bank's  letter  of  credit,  which  effectively  removed 
any  doubt  about  ultimate  recovery  of  the  account.   A  similar  but  dif- 
ferent cue  to  be  found  in  another  debtor's  files  might  refer  to  a 
parent  company's  guarantee  of  indebtedness.   To  avoid  a  proliferation 
of  such  rules  in  this  first  attempt  at  building  an  expert  system  of 
auditor's  judgments,  CORRESPOND  was  formulated  to  allow  the  user  to  sum 
up  in  one  response  the  data  which  he  gathered  from  the  client's  corre- 
spondence files.   Such  a  response  constitutes  in  effect  a  "mini-judgment" 
by  the  user,  which  more  experience  with  the  system  may  suggest  should 
be  divided  into  several  rules.   ECONOMIC  is  also  a  rule  of  rather  fre- 
quent usage,  perhaps  as  a  result  of  concern  about  the  economic  condi- 
tions which  prevailed  when  this  work  was  done  in  (1981  and  1982). 
Usage  of  CREDITMGR  reflects  the  common  audit  practice  of  discussing 
delinquent  accounts  with  a  representative  of  the  client. 

On  the  other  hand,  several  rules  carry  a  relatively  heavy  impact 
but  evidently  are  infrequently  applied.   Auditors  report  they  seldom 
communicate  with  an  attorney  regarding  a  delinquent  account,  but  would 
give  strong  weight  to  a  pessimistic  prediction  which  he  might  make 
(LAWYER).   The  creditor's  stated  intent  not  to  pay  could  represent  an 
important  cue  but  is  rarely  available  (NOTPAY).   The  only  instances 
reported  to  the  researchers  of  the  presence  of  this  cue  occurred  in  the 
case  of  debtors  who  were  disputing  the  propriety  of  the  charges  in  their 
accounts,  claiming  errors  in  pricing.   Similarly,  if  neither  the  audi- 
tor nor  his  client  were  able  to  communicate  with  the  debtor  that  would 
represent  an  important  cue  (NONCONTACT) .   No  such  situation  arose  between 
the  large  commercial  and  government  entities  involved  serving  as  test 
cases  in  the  AUDITOR  studv. 


-18- 


FURTHER  ANALYSIS 

Three  other  aspects  of  the  system  and  its  usage  are  worthy  of  com- 
ment.  Michie  (1980)  and  others  have  suggested  that  an  experienced  prac- 
titioner's expertise  consists  of  his  ability  to  recognize  clusterings 
of  cues — that  is,  patterns — which  he  has  encountered  previously  and 
found  to  be  particularly  diagnostic.   Clusterings  of  cues  may  function 
as  mini-hypotheses,  allowing  the  expert  to  focus  his  evidence-gathering 
in  an  economical  fashion.   To  the  researchers,  the  most  obvious  of  the 
patterns  apparently  utilized  by  auditors  in  the  valuation  of  accounts 
receivable  seems  to  be  organized  around  a  distinction  between  "slow-pay" 
versus  "no-pay"  as  diagnostic  categories  for  each  delinquent  account. 
Thus,  a  slow-pay  customer,  properly  diagnosed,  does  not  require  a  pro- 
vision for  loss.   However,  this  categorization  may  hide  several  patterns 
of  less  global  impact,  which  may  in  reality  be  the  organizers  for  a 
practitioner's  judgment,  such  as  "the  economic-problem"  pattern  or  "the 
legal-problem"  pattern.   This  matter  will  be  explored  in  a  subsequent 
paper . 

The  second  matter  concerns  strength  of  belief  and  the  meaning  of 
AUDITOR'S  report.   AUDITOR  reports  Degrees  of  Belief.   This  constitutes 
its  expert  judgment.   The  user  of  the  system  must  decide  for  himself 
the  significance  of  the  report,  for  example  whether  10  Degrees  (.91 
probability)  mandates  that  an  allowance  be  provided.   The  researchers 
attempted  to  calibrate  the  system  during  its  refinement  stage  so  that 
AUDITOR  would  produce  a  report  of  about  10  Degrees  (positive  or  nega- 
tive), coincident  with  the  time  a  human  expert  auditor  reported  that  he 
had  become  "satisfied"  with  the  data  and  had  made  his  decision.   How- 
ever, many  studies  reflect  unfavorably  on  humankind's  ability  to  pro- 
perly deal  with  data  presented  in  a  probabilistic  format,  c.f.  Joyce 
and  Biddle  (1981a).   Thus,  AUDITOR'S  report  of  Degree  of  Belief  can 
best  be  viewed  as  an  indication  of  relative  strength  of  belief.   No 
claim  is  intended  either  that  AUDITOR'S  report  reflects  objective  prob- 
abilities nor  that  the  researchers  believe  human  experts  process  prob- 
abilities similarly  in  the  construction  of  their  judgments.   However, 
while  keeping  this  disclaimer  well  in  mind,  the  researchers  believe 
that  by  the  time  the  users  terminated  their  validation  sessions  AUDITOR 
can  fairly  be  said  to  have  reached  an  unambiguous  result,  at  least  in 
terms  of  the  refinement  criteria,  in  the  great  majority  of  cases.   For 
example,  for  the  third  client,  which  comprised  the  blind  validation 
trial,  in  only  one  instance  of  eleven  delinquent  debtors  was  the  session 
terminated  when  AUDITOR  was  reporting  less  than  four  percentage  points 
from  a  value  which  would  represent  certainty  (0%  or  100%)  Table  2.   In 
the  ten  open-book  trials,  eight  reported  within  +/-  seven  points, 
results  which  are  within  the  criteria  (Table  1).   Thus,  the  researchers 
believe  this  first  complete  version  of  AUDITOR  can  be  said  to  diagnose 
the  collectibility  of  delinquent  accounts  in  an  unambiguous  fashion, 
which  bodes  well  for  the  development  of  other  expert  systems  for  use 
in  audit  situations. 

The  final  matter  for  discussion  concerns  the  sequence  of  inquiry 
followed  by  the  system.   AUDITOR  asks  next  that  particular  question 


-19- 


which  potentially  has  the  greatest  impact  upon  the  probability  of  the 
hypothesis.   Human  auditors  do  not  organize  their  own  evidence-gathering 
in  such  an  efficient  fashion,  nor  do  they  plan  the  audit  in  order  to 
investigate  every  source  of  highly  reliable  information.   For  example, 
it  is  not  a  standard  audit  procedure  to  request  confirmations  from  every 
large  delinquent  debtor,  even  though  responses  might  reveal  evidence  of 
high  diagnosticity  through  application  of  such  rules  as  PROBLEMS, 
NORESPONSE,  NONCONTACT,  LEGAL,  and  NOTPAY.   Neither  is  it  common  prac- 
tice uniformly  to  inquire  of  the  client's  attorney  or  collection  agency 
concerning  a  delinquent  debtor,  although  evidence  of  high  diagnosticity 
might  result  (LAWYER,  BANKRUPT,  LEGAL,  and  various  interactions). 
Explanation  for  this  apparent  gaffe  is  easily  found.   Auditors  are  cost 
conscious,  AUDITOR  is  not.   This  expert  system  is  designed  to  con- 
centrate on  the  diagnosticity  of  the  evidence  while  ignoring  its  cost. 
Human  expert  auditors,  at  least  those  who  work  for  profit-oriented 
firms,  are  expected  to  be  time  and  cost  conscious. 

Additionally,  of  course,  an  eclectism  in  auditors'  pursuit  of  evi- 
dence is  motivated  by  factors  such  as  the  unpredictable  availability  of 
clients'  records  and  employees,  conflicting  schedules  necessitated  by  a 
desire  to  service  several  clients,  and  a  need  to  accomplish  several  audit 
objectives  in  a  limited  time.   Also,  since  both  client  and  auditor  seek 
more  from  the  audit  than  a  satisfactory  valuation  of  the  allowance  for 
bad  debts,  other  tasks  may  intervene. 

In  only  one  case  during  validation  did  sequencing  of  inquiry  cause 
difficulty.   In  that  case,  #1-2,  the  user  serving  as  validator  ter- 
minated the  validation  session  immediately  prior  to  a  question  which 
had  great  bearing  on  that  particular  debtor  and  which  would  have  put 
AUDITOR  more  in  agreement  with  the  judgment  of  the  human  auditor,  that 
is,  at  a  higher  Degree  and  probability  level.   In  all  other  instances, 
the  sequence  of  greatest-impact-first,  with  termination  controlled  by 
the  user,  appeared  to  be  a  satisfactory  method  of  handling  the  evidence 
contained  in  working  papers  without  provoking  a  complaint  by  the  user 
of  the  system.   Thus,  the  sequencing  in  AUDITOR  seems  to  be  at  least 
satisfactory  to  its  users  in  the  majority  of  cases.   Whether  it  is  rea- 
sonable to  expect  auditors  to  follow  a  more  efficient  search  pattern  is 
beyond  the  scope  of  this  paper. 


SUMMARY 


Expert  systems  such  as  AUDITOR  can  constitute  a  model  of  auditors' 
judgment,  answering  the  researchable  and  interesting  question  said  to 
be  at  the  heart  of  all  investigation  of  diagnostic  judgment  in  the  face 
of  uncertainty:   what  cues  do  the  judges  use,  what  are  their  weights, 
and  how  are  they  assembled  into  a  judgment  model.   Thus,  subject  to  the 
influence  of  the  researchers'  intervention,  AUDITOR  demonstrates  the 
feasibility  of  a  new  approach  to  the  descriptive  study  of  auditors' 
judgments . 


-20- 


LIMITATIONS 

All  of  the  auditors  who  participated  in  the  initial  system-building 
were  members  of  one  office  of  one  large  public  accounting  firm.   To  a 
great  extent  all  had  been  exposed  to  similar  professional  training. 
Additionally,  they  perform  under  policies  which  might  perhaps  be  unique 
to  one  firm.   However,  auditors  from  other  firms  were  involved  in  the 
refinement  and  validation  stages  of  the  project — which  lends  confidence 
in  the  universality  of  the  system  as  a  model  of  auditors'  judgment  and 
reduces  the  chances  of  parochialism. 

Further  refinement  of  the  system  may  produce  better  results.   Rules 
might  be  refined  to  reduce  them  more  nearly  to  the  elemental  cues, 
reducing  the  need  for  the  mini-judgments  presently  contained  in  such 
rules  as  CORRESPOND  and  LEGAL.   Such  a  revised  rule  base  might  be  more 
reliably  applied  by  an  unexperienced  auditor — one  who  had  not  developed 
competence  in  the  mini-judgments  which  a  few  of  the  rules  call  for.   On 
the  other  hand,  such  changes  increase  the  size  of  the  rule  base  and 
lengthen  the  list  of  questions  to  which  a  user  is  exposed — factors  which 
tend  to  decrease  the  convenience  of  working  with  the  system.   However, 
work  papers  used  by  the  auditors  who  were  involved  in  various  stages  of 
this  work  evidently  contained  very  little  relevant  data  beyond  that 
called  for  in  the  rules  of  the  system,  with  the  exception  of  the  age  of 
the  delinquent  balance  which  was  under  scrutiny.   Some  indication  during 
validation  was  gained  that  the  absolute  age  of  the  account  may  have  to 
some  auditors  a  significance  not  reflected  in  the  system.   Therefore, 
a  rule  relating  to  age  of  the  delinquent  balance  might  be  useful  in  the 
system.   However,  for  Che  large,  successful,  client  companies  whose 
audit  work  papers  were  referred  to  in  various  stages  of  this  work  it 
seemed  likely  that  no  delinquent  account  was  allowed  to  gain  more  than 
a  few  months'  age  before  some  finality  was  forced  in  its  disposition. 
(These  can  be  presumed  to  be  clients  with  relatively  good  internal  con- 
trols over  sales  and  collections.)   However,  there  is  the  possibility 
that  alternative  systems  of  rules  and  weights  may  produce  equally  suc- 
cessful systems  which  demonstrate  audit  judgment. 

The  influence  and  beliefs  of  the  observer — the  researchers — cannot 
be  ruled  out  in  project  of  this  kind.   Also,  the  subjects'  desires  to 
please  and  accommodate  the  researchers  may  work  to  the  detriment  of 
effort  to  create  and  particularly  to  validate  an  effective  expert  system. 

Criticism  can  be  directed  against  the  system  for  its  use  of  Bayes ' 
revision.   It  is  commonly  believed  that  auditors  are  no  more  Bayesian 
in  their  processing  of  evidence  than  are  other  humans  (Libby,  1981). 
However,  the  point  is  worth  emphasizing  that  the  use  of  Bayesian  revi- 
sion in  computerized  expert  systems  operating  in  fields  other  than 
auditing  produce  results  similar  enough  to  those  produced  by  human 
experts  as  to  satisfy  these  experts  themselves. 

Further  refinement  of  the  system  may  be  called  for,  particularly, 
regarding  instances  in  which  practicing  auditors  conclude  that  partial 
rather  than  100%  allowances  are  proper  to  provide  against  a  delinquent 


-^1  - 


account.   In  the  Open-Book  Validation  procedure,  auditors  called  for  a 
partial  allowance  against  an  account  for  which  AUDITOR'S  processing 
produced  (upon  replay  by  the  researcher)  a  result  of  approximately  70%. 
Further  experience  with  the  system  may  serve  to  clarify  the  set  of  cir- 
cumstances under  which  human  experts  call  for  partial  allowance  and  the 
probabilities  which  AUDITOR  might  be  expected  to  report  in  the  same 
circumstances. 

On  the  other  hand,  the  judgments  of  expert  auditors  may  be  suf- 
ficiently idiosyncratic  and  inconsistent  that  attempts  to  refine  the 
system  any  further  will  be  fruitless.   Changes  to  the  system  will  be 
carried  out  only  after  an  analysis  of  the  objective  to  be  sought.   For 
example,  if  enhanced  convenience  of  the  system  for  use  by  practicing 
auditors  were  to  be  an  objective  of  modification,  a  desirable  change 
might  be  to  engineer  a  revision  in  the  order  in  which  certain  questions 
are  asked.   Certain  evidence,  while  of  lower  diagnostic  impact,  seems 
always  to  be  available,  and  might  be  asked  about  early  in  a  consultation 
to  allow  the  user  promptly  to  contribute  his  knowledge  early  in  ques- 
tioning.  On  the  other  hand,  already  existing  capabilities  within  AL/X 
permit  the  user  to  volunteer  evidence  prior  to  questioning.   For  example, 
if  no  request  for  confirmation  was  sent  and  no  information  was  received 
from  any  outside  source,  the  system  could  be  modified  to  accept  this 
data  even  before  questioning  began.   These  capabilities  have  not  been 
exercised  yet  in  AUDITOR. 


-22- 

FOOTNOTES 

Named  second  in  frequency  was  the  judgment  process  leading  to  the 
budget  of  audit  time,  the  study  of  which  might  have  allowed  useful  com- 
parisons with  previous  research  (c.f.  Joyce,  1976).   However,  the  widely 
differing  descriptions  of  this  process  which  were  given  by  the  experts 
discouraged  the  researchers  from  attempting  to  extract  their  expertise. 
The  judgment  process  involved  in  determining  the  threshhold  for  "report- 
ing materiality"  was  similarly  considered  then  rejected  for  study  because 
the  aspects  of  negotiation  evidently  resorted  to  between  client  and 
auditor  have  not  been  amenable  to  expert  system  technology. 

2 
AL/X  (Advice  Language  /  X)  was  developed  for  Intelligent  Terminals 

Ltd.  by  John  Reiter,  Steve  Barth,  and  Andy  Paterson  in  association  with 
the  University  of  Edinburgh  and  was  supported  by  BP  Petroleum  Develop- 
ment Ltd.   It  is  a  Pascal  system  based  upon  the  Prospector  consultant 
system  for  mineral  exploration  developed  at  SRI  International  by  Richard 
Duda,  Peter  Hart,  and  others,  see  Duda  et  al.  (1979). 

3 
The  capabilities  of  AL/X  allow  formation  of  queries  in  a  form 

calling  for  a  "Yes"  or  "No"  answer.   This  capability  was  installed  after 

the  work  on  AUDITOR  had  begun.   Because  of  the  apparent  ease  with  which 

users  of  the  system  had  already  adapted  their  responses  to  the  CV  scale, 

this  added  capability  was  not  used  in  AUDITOR. 

RESERVE  does  not  make  provision  for  situations  in  which  the  audi- 
tor concludes  the  amount  to  be  provided  for  potential  loss  of  the  account 
should  be  more  than  zero  but  less  than  100%  of  the  delinquent  balance. 
One  such  case  was  encountered  among  the  twenty-one  delinquent  accounts 
reviewed  during  validation  of  the  system,  and  the  validator  ruled  that 
AUDITOR  did  not  perform  satisfactorily. 

Four  separate  sets  of  cues  were  accumulated  from  these  experts. 
The  nature  of  differences  between  these  cue  sets  will  be  explored  in  a 
later  paper.   The  final  AUDITOR  system  was  an  aggregation  of  the  cues 
provided  by  the  four  experts. 

Rules  related  by  AND  linkages  are  processed  according  to  the  "fuzzy 
logic"  rules  of  L.  Zadeh  (1979):   P(A1  AND  A2  AND... AND  An)  =  minimum 
[P(A1),  P(A2) , . . . ,P(An) ] ,  where  P  is  probability  and  the  An  are  eviden- 
tial cues. 

It  was  also  necessary  to  "prime"  the  system  by  providing  the 
Bayesian  portions  of  it  with  initial  (prior)  degrees  of  belief.   In  the 
absence  of  any  definitive  guidance  in  the  literature  or  from  the  experts 
all  such  initial  values  were  set  to  reflect  0.0  degrees  of  belief,  which 
is  a  probability  of  0.5.   At  the  beginning  of  any  session  with  AUDITOR 
these  values  can  be  changed  if  desired. 


-23- 

g 
In  both  open-book  and  blind  validation  procedures  the  assumption 

was  made  that  the  work:  papers  contained  both  the  auditors'  judgments  and 
the  support  therefore,  in  conformity  with  Generally  Accepted  Auditing 
Standards,  AU  338. 

9 
Since  AUDITOR  reports  its  results  in  degrees  of  belief  and  in  prob- 
abilities, which  would  be  a  novel  way  for  a  subjective  audit  judgment 
to  be  recorded  in  work  papers,  it  was  necessary  for  the  researchers  to 
interpret  and  in  effect  to  translate  AUDITOR'S  report  before  its  presen- 
tation to  the  validator  in  order  to  maintain  anonymity  of  the  source  of 
each  judgment  appearing  on  the  Comparison  Worksheet.   This  interpretation 
was  easily  made  in  all  but  one  case.   In  every  case  but  one,  AUDITOR'S 
report  produced  a  DB  of  at  least  15,  positive  or  negative,  that  is, 
probability  of  at  least  .97  either  in  favor  of  or  against  the  hypothesis 
which  called  for  an  allowance.   These  results  appeared  to  the  researchers 
to  be  unequivocal  and  were  reported  on  the  Comparison  Worksheet  as  that 
of  an  expert  auditor  who  called  for  "No  reserve  nor  adjustment  needed," 
(in  the  cases  which  reported  negative  DBs)  or  "Yes,  reserve  or  adjust- 
ment needed"  (in  cases  which  reported  positive  DBs).   (In  keeping  with 
common  practice  the  term  "reserve"  was  freely  used  as  a  synonym  for 
allowance  for  bad  debts  among  the  researchers  and  the  practitioners.) 
However,  one  case,  that  of  number  7,  produced  a  DB  of  2  (probability 
.61) — an  equivocal  result  having  neither  precedent  nor  clear  interpre- 
tation within  the  researchers'  experience  with  the  AUDITOR  system.   The 
researchers  reported  this  on  the  Comparison  Worksheet  as,  "At  most  a 
partial  reserve  or  adjustment  is  required."   On  the  same  case,  the  audit 
team  had  found  no  need  for  an  allowance;  the  validator  scored  this  judg- 
ment at  a  hit.   The  validator  initially  scored  AUDITOR'S  result  as  a 
miss  and  that  is  the  way  it  was  recorded  by  the  researchers.   Later, 
however,  when  reviewing  his  work,  he  referred  to  AUDITOR'S  result  on 
number  7  as  a  "close  call."   The  issue  of  partial  allowance  will  be 
studied  further  in  later  applications  of  AUDITOR. 

This  cue  contributes  to  the  researchers '  impression  that  the 
expert  auditors'  basic  diagnostic  task  consists  of  an  attempt  to  clas- 
sify each  delinquent  account  as  "slow-pay"  or  "no-pay,"  that  is,  these 
categories  constitute  competing  hypotheses  for  classification  of  each 
delinquent  account. 

Technical  data  for  this  appendix  also  came  from  "AL/X  USER  MANUAL," 
Andy  Paterson,  Intelligent  Terminals  LTD.   Oxford,  England,  1981. 


-24- 


APPENDIX  A:   Description  of  AL/X  and  AUDITOR11 


The  structure  of  an  expert  system,  in  general,  and  AL/X,  in  parti- 
cular, parallels  the  structure  of  human  decision  making.   Data  is 
gathered,  analyzed,  evaluated  with  respect  to  some  criteria  and  used, 
and,  if  more  data  is  required,  what  additional  data  to  gather  is  deter- 
mined.  Figure  A-l  shows  how  the  specific  step  in  the  operation  of  AL/X 
map  to  the  generic  decision  making  phases. 

The  two  key  operational  areas  are  revision  and  selection.   Revision 
refers  to  the  process  of  updating  the  Degree  of  Belief  associated  with 
the  spaces,  that  is,  the  rules  and  hypotheses.   Figure  A- 2  depicts  this 
revision  as  a  two  stage  process.   At  all  times  the  hypothesis,  which  is 
the  goal  of  the  system's  inquiry  process,  has  associated  with  it  a 
Degree  of  Belief  (DB)  or  simply  degree.   Degrees  of  Belief  measure 
strength  of  belief  on  a  scale  having  a  range  from  -100  to  +100  but, 
AUDITOR  only  uses  a  range  of  -30  to  +30.   The  initial  ("prior")  DB 
value  within  AUDITOR  of  the  hypothesis,  RESERVE,  is  set  to  0.0  but  can 
be  easily  set  to  any  initial  value.   The  DB  scale  is  derived  from  prob- 
abilities as: 


Degree  of  Hypothesis(H)  =  10  log10(Probability(H)/(l-Probability(H)) 

Similarly,  every  space  carries  a  DB  which  reflects  its  prior  strength 
of  belief.   The  user's  response  (CV)  to  each  question  initiates  stage  1; 
revision  immediately  of  the  DB  for  that  space  (unless  the  response  is  0) . 
The  value  of  CV  revises  the  degree  in  the  following  manner.   A  user's 
response  of  +5  assigns  to  that  space  the  maximum  DB  permitted  by  the 
systems,  i.e.,  +100.   Similarly,  an  answer  of  -5  results  in  a  degree  of 
-100.   An  answer  of  zero  leaves  the  degree  unchanged  at  its  prior  value. 
An  answer  other  than  +5,  -5  or  0  results  in  interpolations  between  these 
three  points.   A  linear  interpolation  is  performed  on  probability  values. 
A  positive  CV  causes  interpolation  between  the  prior  probability  and  1. 
A  negative  CV  value  results  in  interpolation  between  zero  and  the  prior 
probability.   The  resulting  probability  value  is  then  converted  back  to 
a  degree  value.   The  effects  of  the  user's  answer  are  immediately  pro- 
pagated through  the  inference  net  to  update  the  DB  of  all  spaces  for 
which  the  selected  question  is  evidence,  including  the  hypotheses. 

The  second  stage  of  the  two-stage  process  of  revision  involves  cal- 
culations of  the  incremental  weight  which  will  be  added  to  the  present 
degree  of  the  hypothesis  (and  to  the  present  degree  of  any  other  spaces 
which,  in  a  particular  system,  might  be  consequences  of  the  immediate 
question  to  which  the  user  is  responding).   First,  we  will  speak  about 
revisions  involving  spaces  connected  via  IF:THEN  inferences.   A  process 
of  interpolation  is  again  used  employing  the  PW  or  the  NW  of  the  space, 
together  with  the  degree  calculated  from  the  user's  CV  response.   If 
the  evidence  is  true  then  a  positive  weight  (PW)  of  one  will  increase 
the  degree  of  belief  of  the  hypothesis  by  one.   Similarly  if  the  evidence 


-25- 


GATHER  DATA 


£ 


SYSTEM 

ASKS 

QUESTION 


USER'S 

RESPONSE 

(CV) 


ANALYZE 


REVISION 


UPDATE   DI 


"1 
[REPORT  TCl 

USER 


EVALUATE 


DETERMINE  NEXT  QUESTION 


MORE 
UESTION 


SYSTEM  SELECTS 
NEXT  QUESTION 


( STOPj 


Figure  A-l  Overview  of  Expert  Systems 


-26- 


USER'S 

RESPONSE 

ON 

CV  SCALE 

r 


TRANSFORMATION 
OF  CV  TO 
PROBABILITY 
EQUIVALENT 


CONVERSION 
OF  PROBABILITY 
EQUIVALENT  TO 
DEGREE  OF  BELIEF 
(  DB) 


|_R  EV I  SIO  N_:  STAGE  J, 


r 


____j 


PW   OR  NW 
OF  SPACE 
V/HICH  IS 
CAUSING  REVISION 


1 


CALCULATION   OF 
INCREMENTAL 
WEIGHT  (  IVV  ) 

IW=f(DB,PW,NW) 


HYPOTHESIS 

NEW    DB= 
OLD  DB  +  IW 


j  REVISION:  S  TAG  E_2 


j 


Figure  A-2  Overview  of  the  Process  of  Revision 


-27- 


is  false  and  the  negative  weight  (NW)  of  one  then  the  degree  of  the  hypo- 
thesis will  decrease  by  one.   The  greater  the  positive  weight  then  the 
more  strongly  does  the  presence  of  the  evidence  imply  that  the  hypothesis 
is  true.   For  negative  weights,  the  corresponding  implication  is  that 
the  absence  of  the  evidence  implies  that  the  hypothesis  is  false.   In 
the  situation  where  the  presence  of  evidence  implies  that  a  hypothesis 
is  false  rather  than  true  it  is  perfectly  legitimate  to  have  a  negative, 
positive  weight  and  a  positive,  negative  weight  (i.e.,  PW  =  -4  and  NW  = 
+5). 

If  the  degree  of  the  space,  as  a  result  of  the  user's  response  is 
greater  than  the  prior  degree  of  that  space,  then  the  incremental  weight 
to  be  added  to  the  degree  of  the  hypothesis  is  calculated  as 


Incremental  weight  =  PW/(PW*  min(PW,  current  -  prior)) 


If  the  degree  of  the  space  as  calculated  is  less  than  the  prior  degree 
of  that  space,  then  the  incremental  weight  is: 


Incremental  weight  =  NW/(NW*  min(NW,  prior  -  current)). 


AND  spaces  are  composed  of  the  connection  by  AND  of  two  or  more  IFrTHEN 
spaces.   Each  AND  space  has  its  own  degree  of  belief.   Also,  each  AND 
space  has  its  own  PW  and  NW.   The  weight  which  an  AND  space  has  on  the 
hypothesis  is  dependent  on  the  degree  of  that  AND  space  and  its  PW  and 
NW,  just  as  if  it  were  an  IFrTHEN  space.   The  degree  of  an  AND  space  is 
a  function  of  the  degree  of  the  components  and  is  calculated  as 


degree  (Al  AND  A2  AND  ...  AND  An)  =  min(DB(Al),  DB(A2),  ...,  DB(An)) 


Selection  refers  to  the  manner  in  which  AL/X  chooses  which,  of  many, 
questions  is  appropriate  to  ask  next.   (See  Figure  A-3).   The  manner  in 
which  this  is  done  is  important  because  human  experts  are  characterized 
by  their  ability  to  focus  quickly  on  the  most  efficient  line  of  ques- 
tioning.  AL/X  chooses  and  investigates  that  particular  hypothesis  which 
is  most  likely  out  of  a  group  of  perhaps  several  alternative  hypotheses. 
The  choice  criterion  is:   Select  that  hypothesis  which  currently  has 
the  highest  degree  of  belief.   Once  the  hypothesis  has  been  chosen, 
questions  are  selected  which  will  speed  resolution  of  the  diagnosis. 
In  AUDITOR,  the  process  of  selection  consists  of  choosing  which  ques- 
tion to  ask  of  the  user,  i.e.,  which  question  has  the  highest  potential 
incremental  weight  impact  on  the  hypothesis.   The  incremental  weight  of 
each  of  the  remaining  questions  is  calculated  prospectively  by  the 
system.   AL/X  carries  this  out  by  scanning  through  all  of  the,  as  yet, 
unanswered  questions,  calculating  the  incremental  weights  which  could 


-28- 


PW    AND    NW 

OF  UNANSWERED 

QUESTIONS 


PW   AND    NW 
OF   AND 
SPACES 


SYSTEM   ASKS 
QUESTION 


USER 
RESPONDS 


SYSTEM 
CALCULATES 

.IW*OF  ALL 
UNANSWERED 

QUESTIONS 


SYSTEM 
IDENTIFIES 
UNANSWERED 
QUESTION   WITH 
HIGHEST   IW 


IW=   incremental   v/eight 


Figure  A-3  Overview  of  the  Method  for  Selecting  the  Next  Question 


-29- 


arise  from  each  question,  as  a  result  of  a  CV  response  by  the  user  of 
either  -5  or  +5.   (AND  and  NOT  spaces  are  included  in  the  calculation.) 
That  question  is  chosen  next  which  could  conceivably  cause  the  greatest 
impact  on  the  strength  of  belief  of  the  hypothesis.   Since  the  poten- 
tial impact  of  an  AND  space  depends  upon  the  user's  answers  to  the  com- 
ponent spaces,  the  order  of  questioning  may  vary  from  one  consultation 
to  the  next  depending  upon  the  user's  responses  to  the  components  of 
the  AND  spaces.   This  knowledge  is  utilized  by  the  system  in  the  form 
of  rules  often  in  the  style  I_F: Evidence ,  THEN  (to  a  specified 
extent ) :Hypothesis .   The  connectors  AND,  OR,  and  NOT  may  also  be  used. 

The  data  are  immediately  incorporated  into  the  system  by  the  con- 
trol and  processed  through  the  rule  base.  This  action  updates  the  rule 
base  to  reflect  the  user's  knowledge  and  revises  the  degree  of  belief 
in  the  hypothesis  which  has  been  the  focus  of  the  expert's  inquiry  pro- 
cess. It  is  the  degree  of  belief  in  the  hypothesis  (and  its  transla- 
tion into  probabilities)  which  constitutes  the  goal  of  the  system  and 
its  expert  judgment. 


Goals  with  degree  >  0.0  are: 

The  delinquent  portion  of  this  account  should  specifically  be  reserved 
for  in  the  allowance  for  bad  debts  to  a  substantial  extent  (RESERVE). 
Prior  degree  was  0.0.   Current  degree  is  13.8.   At  this  point  the  goal 
is  certain:   Probability  .95  or  greater. 


The  above  is  AUDITOR'S  report  that  the  user's  answers  to  its  ques- 
tions have  resulted  in  increasing  the  strength  of  belief  of  the  truth 
of  the  hypothesis  called  RESERVE  from  0.0  to  13.8  on  the  degree  (of 
belief)  scale.   Prior  to  questioning  of  the  user,  RESERVE  is  presumed 
as  likely  to  be  true  as  false,  that  is,  to  have  prior  degree  (or  belief) 
of  0.0.   A  degree  of  belief  (DB)  or,  simply,  degree  measures  strength 
of  belief  in  the  goal  hypothesis.   The  possible  range  is  from  -100  to 
100.   Since  AUDITOR'S  report  is  based  upon  its  Bayesian  processing  of 
subjective  probabilities,  the  researchers  utilized  the  report  of 
degrees  and  probabilities  merely  as  a  guide  to  imputing  a  judgment  from 
AUDITOR,  and  initially  operated  under  the  assumption  that  a  probability 
of  .90  (degree  of  9.5)  would  indicate  the  need  for  an  allowance. 

A  degree  of  zero  indicates  that  the  hypothesis  is  equally  likely 
and  unlikely,  i.e.,  probability  of  0.5.   The  mid-range  of  the  degree 
scale  from  about  -10  to  10  DB  represents  a  large  range  of  probability 
(about  .10  to  .90).   Thus,  small  increments  in  degree  may  represent 
large  changes  in  probability.   This  effect  is  considered  desirable  in 
this  expert  system  since  small  changes  in  degree  are  considered  to  be 


-30- 


relatively  more  diagnostic  when  the  current  truth  or  falsity  of  the 
hypothesis  lies  in  a  doubtful  or  ambiguous  area. 

Only  rarely,  however,  are  the  cue  weights  symmetrical.   For  example, 
the  rule  stated  as,  "This  debtor  is  in  bankruptcy-type  proceedings" 
(BANKRUPT)  carries  PW  =  3.0,  NW  =  -1.0.   These  values  indicate  that  to 
the  experts  the  debtor's  bankruptcy  is  considerably  more  diagnostic — 
reflected  by  the  PW  =  3.0 — of  uncollectibility  than  the  absence  of  bank- 
ruptcy is  predictive  of  collectibility. 

The  reader  may  notice  two  consequences  of  these  relationships. 
First,  an  apparently  small  portion  of  the  available  range  on  the  DB 
scale,  say  from  about  -10  to  10,  encompasses  the  rather  large  proba- 
bility range  .09  to  .91.   Additionally,  since  the  effect  of  an  eviden- 
tial cue  is  additive  upon  the  hypothesis,  the  diagnostic  or  inferential 
strength  of  any  piece  of  evidence  is  at  its  highest  when  it  impacts  an 
hypothesis  of  neutral  degree  and  decreases  as  more  evidence  is  accumu- 
lated.  This  is  believed  to  be  a  desirable  feature  in  an  expert  system. 


-31- 


APPENDIX  B: 

ILLUSTRATION  OF  AUDITOR'S  OPERATION 


A  hypothetical  case  situation  is  used  to  demonstrate  the  operation 
of  the  system.   First,  the  facts  of  the  situation,  as  they  might  be  in 
the  working  papers,  are  described.   Second,  the  consultative  session 
between  AUDITOR  and  the  expert  is  shown,  including  annotations  to  high- 
light the  features  of  AUDITOR.   And,  third,  the  results  of  the  AUDITOR 
session  are  given  in  terms  of  degrees  of  belief  (DB)  and  probability, 
which  represent  the  revised  likelihood  of  the  hypothesis  under  scrutiny. 


SITUATION 

The  delinquent  amount  owed  by  this  customer,  a  regional,  publicly 
held  hardware  chain,  is  $82,000.   This  represents  about  two-thirds  of 
the  balance  of  one  invoice  approximately  75  days  overdue  on  which  the 
customer  has  recently  paid  $40,000.   In  total,  the  customer  owes 
$320,000,  the  rest  of  it  classified  as  current  (45  days  and  under). 
The  overdue  amount,  while  large  among  delinquent  accounts,  is  clearly 
not  material  to  the  client's  financial  statements.   They  show  accounts 
receivable  of  $20  million  and  current  assets  of  $100  million.   The 
accounts  receivable  subsidiary  ledger  shows  the  most  recent  three-year 
history  of  the  account,  in  which  the  customer  has  become  overdue  only 
one  other  time.   He  ultimately  paid  the  full  amount  owed  after  an 
adjustment  was  made  which  represented  about  2%  of  the  invoice.   Sales 
this  year  are  above  those  of  a  year  ago,  probably  reflecting  a  trend 
toward  "do-it-yourself"  repairs  by  hardware  customers.   Correspondence 
and  other  files  indicate  a  long-term,  on-going  business  relationship 
with  no  suggestion  of  any  problems  with  the  account. 

The  auditors  did  not  attempt  to  confirm  the  account  and  there  is  no 
information  available  from  an  attorney  or  any  other  source  outside  of 
the  client.   The  customer  is  not  bankrupt.   The  client  continues  to 
make  credit  sales  to  this  customer,  who  has  paid  other  invoices  within 
their  due  dates.   According  to  the  credit  manager  who  has  talked  with 
them,  the  customer  states  emphatically  he  will  not  pay  the  balance 
which  is  overdue.   The  credit  manager  goes  on  to  say,  "Sometimes  we 
give  them  a  small  adjustment  when  they  put  up  a  fuss  like  this,  but 
they  have  been  good  customers  for  15  years  and  they  always  pay."   The 
"fuss"  referred  to  in  this  case  evidently  resulted  from  a  late  delivery 


CONSULTATIVE  SESSION 

The  user  first  signs  onto  the  system.   AUDITOR  then  begins  ques- 
tioning the  user  about  the  facts  of  the  case.   The  order  of  the  ques- 
tions depends  on  the  user's  responses.   Material  in  brackets  represents 
AUDITOR  output,  in  parentheses  the  user's  responses,  and  in  asterisks 
the  researchers  annotations.   The  result  of  this  session  is  that  the 


-32- 


systera  determines  that  there  is  only  .013  probability  (-19.5  DB)  that 
an  allowance  need  be  provided  for  this  account.   Other,  similar  accounts 
would  go  through  a  similar  set  of  interactions. 


■33- 


APPENDIX  C:   NETWORK  DESCRIPTION  FILE 


MODEL  3ADDE3T 
VERSION  A3DMA 

SPACE  RESERVE 

TEXT  DESCRIPTION 
/*  THE  DELINQUENT  PORTION  OF  THIS  ACCOUNT  SHOULD  SPECIFICALLY  3E  RESEEVED-FOR  IN 
THE  ALLOWANCE  FOR  3 AD  DE3TS  TO  A  SUBSTANTIAL  DEGREE  */ 

INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

SPACE  OUTSTAND 

TEXT  DESCRIPTION 
/*  WHILE  SMALL  PAYMENTS  ARE  3EING  RECEIVED  ON  THIS  ACCOUNT.   THE  OUTSTANDING 
DELINQUENT  BALANCE  IS  GROWING  LARGER  */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES  CONSEQUENTS  (  RESERVE  PW  1  NW  0  )  . 

SPACE  AVEAG2 

TEXT  DESCRIPTION 
/*  THE  AVERAGE  AGE  OF  THE  UNCOLLECTED  PORTIONS  OF  THIS  ACCOUNT  IS  INCREASING  */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES   CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE   PW  0.5    NW   -0.5    ) 

SPACE   ALL3UT0NE 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION 
/*    THIS    CUSTOMER'S    ACCOUNT   3ALANCS    IS    ALL   CURRENT    EXCEPT    FCR  ONE    I^RGS   AND 
DELINQUENT  OiJ-JRGZ    */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES  CONSEQUENTS  (  RESERVE  FW  0.5  NW  0  ) 

SPACE  AL3T1&NOT? 

TEXT  DESCRIPTION 
/*  ALL3UT0NE  AND  NOTPAY  */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

LOGICAL  DEFINITION  AND  (  ALL3UTCNE  NOTPAY  ) 

RULES  CONSEQUENTS  (  RESERVE  PW  2.0  NW  0  ) 

SPACE  CREDITS TOP 

TEXT  DESCRIPTION 
/*  CREDIT  TO  THIS  CUSTOMER  HAS  3EEN  STOPPED  BY  THE  CLIENT  */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE   PW   1    NW    -0 . 5    ) 


-34- 


4.  Kac!  been  assigned  to  a  collection  age,.cy  or 
C.-that  this  account  ha^  DeB"  °^   a 

lawyer?   ^3  (-5) 

C   .that  this  debtor  has  a  ^V"^""^!*  U>timately  payi"9 
^stantially  all  delinquent  balances?  71     (5) 

«.,-  •,,««■  ever  been  received  from  this  customer? 
C.bhat  no  payments  havs  ever  u 

?3  (-5)  ' 

*.~  «r.  h^d  stated  his  intent  to  pay  little  op 
C.that  the  customer  haS  sw  ,  C.that  despite  tha 

nothing  of  the   e hnquent ^  ance s       .^  ^  ^  ^ny 

presence  of    «  deh  ^    3uditor's  call  to  the  system  to 
paid?   ?1  (PG)  *  PG   is  Li         either  than  an  adequate  amount  of 
present  its  conclusion.  £ailable  Qvidence.  has  been  entered  into 
evidence*  or  all  of  the  3v<s* 
the  system.* 

•«««««""**"""to  dea^e  cf  belief  >  -100.0  are:  .-..none  at 
Clnvest19ated  goals  with  deg  indicating  that  the  goal 

the  moment. 3  *This  should  ^e  r  other  words,  thera 

hypothesis  has  not  been  £hUX ™v£   ^     of  the  system  prefers 
are  still  questions  "nasked^  but  ^  ^^  y^  .fc  £g 

that  they  not  be  asked.  Also,  t,    V         uheneVar  '.t  is  over 


for  the  hypothesis  whenever 


t 


set  to  report  the  degree 
-100  DB-* 

COther  goals  with  degree  >  ~100  areS 

u •  „  ~r  this  account  should  specifically  bs 
The  delinquent  Pj;  ^,   a^Sfor  bad  debts  to  a  substantial 

reserved- for  in  tne  aixuv 
degree  :RESERVe. 

n  a   rurrent  degree  is  -19.5.   At  this  point  this 
Prior  degree  was  0 . 0  .  cui  /      |       probt3bility  is  only. 013 
goal  is  P^^i1^-9^;' ,ccount  will  become  uncollectible: 
SP:;l';  user  will  Undoubtedly  conclude  no  provision  need  b, 
rr.ade  for  loss  from  this  account.* 

^jc********  *************************************** 
««*«******-«************** 

.  fh.t  despite  the  presence  of  this  delinquent 
CHow  certamareyo    hat  £ esp         P      ^  #  ^  ^^      ^ 

item,  newer  items  have bj.«n *    y  P       unanswered,  and  the  user^ 
rp3pat5  the  last  question  wnx.ul  .  .f       •.  •     •  ~    * 
?!   !u      *    ^^  r-^^e  or  Quit  its  questionn  ing.* 
tells  the  system  to  ceasF? 


■35- 


CThe  current  goal  is  whether  cr  not  the  ce] inquent  portion  of  this 
account  should  specifically  be  reserved-for  in  the  allowance  for 
bad  debts  to  a  substantial  degree.  RESERVE!.  *7he  system  is 
announcing  its  goal  hypothesis.* 

CHow  certain  are  you  that  your  te"sts  of  cash  receipts  indicate 
that  the  delinquent  portions  oP  the  account  in  question  have  been 
collected  substantially  in  their  entirety  subsequent  to  the  date 
of  the  balance  sheet?   ?3  (-5)  *0n  this  line  the  system  has  given 
a  "prompt"  in  the  form  of  a  question  mark ■  and  the  auditor  has 
replied  with  "-5"  which  indicates  he  is  certain  the  account  haa 
not  been  substantially  collected.* 

CHow  certain  are  you  that  serious  problems  with  the  delinquent 
portions  of  this  account  were  revealed  through  confirmation 
causing  you  to  believe  they  are  invalid? 

?3   (0)  *No  confirmation  was  requested*  so  the  user  responds  with  a 
"O"  which  means*  in  this  case*  the  question  is  irrelevant.* 

C...that  legal  counsel  gives  poor  prospects  of  any  significant 
recovery  from  this  debtor?   ?3  (0)  #There  is  no  information 
available  from  a  lawyer.* 

C...that  the  confirmation  request  was  returned  by  the  postal 
service  as  undel iverable  and  the  client  for  several  months  has 
been  unable  to  communicate  with  the  debtor?  71      (0)  -*The  auditor 
responded  to  this  question  as  irrelevant.* 

• 

C...that  recent  correspondence  and  other  data  in  the  customer's 
credit  file  supports  your  belief  in  the  collectibility  of  this 
account?  71     <5)  *The  auditor  believes  the  long-term  business 
relationship  will  be  continued  by  the  customer*  or  that  for  other 
reasons  the  account  will  by  collected.* 

C...that  recent  collections  toward  the  delinquent  portions  of  thits 
account  are  proceeding  satisfactorily?  71     (5)  *A  substantial 
amount  has  recently  been  received.* 

C...that  the  credit  manager  expresses  a  strong  belief  in  the 
ultimate  collection  of  substantially  all  this  account?   ?3  (5) 

C...that  although  a  portion  of  this  customer's  total  balance  is 
still  delinquent*  he  continues  to  be  an  active  customer?  71     (5) 

C...that  this  debtor  is  in  bankruptcy-type  proceedings?  71      (-5) 

C...that  economic  factors  are  causing  particularly  bad  effects  on 
this  customer's  3bility  to  pay?  71     (-5) 

C...that  the  .r.erits  of  this  debtor's  likely  counterclaim  agiansfc 
your  client's  suit  indicate  that  a  legal  action  would  be 
fruitless?   °3   (0)  *The  auditor  has  no  information  about  a 

counterclaim.* 

*» 


-36- 


SPACS    NRES&NTACT 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION 
/*    NORESPONSE  AND  NOT  ACTIVE    */ 
i .«-  ERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

LOGICAL  DEFINITION  AND  (  NORESPCNSE  NOTACTIVE  ) 

RULES  CONSEQUENTS  (  RESERVE  PW  2.0  NW  -1.0  ) 

SPACE  NONCONTACT 

text  description 

/*  the  conftrmatioh  request  was  retorned  3y  the  postal  service  as  cndelivsra3le  and 
the  client  for  several  Months  has  been  unable  to  communicate  with  the  debtor  */ 

inference 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE   PW   4.5    NW  0.0    ) 

SPACE    NCNT&NOTACT 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION  -     - 

/*    NONCONTACT   AND   NOTACTIVE    */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

LOGICAL   DEFINITION   AND    (    NONCONTACT   NOTACTIVE    ) 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE  PW   5.0   NW  0.0    ) 

SPACE   NCTPAY 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION 
/*   THE   CUSTOMER  HAS    STATED  HIS    INTENT  TO  PAY  LITTLE   OR  NOTHING  OF   THE   DELINQUENT 
BALANCES    */ 

INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE  PW    3.0    NW   -1.0    ) 

SPACE   BANKRUPT 

TEXT  DESCRIPTION  ■ 
/*  THIS   DE3T0R   IS    IN   3ANXRUPTCY-TYFS   PROCEEDINGS    */ 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES  CONSEQUENTS  (  RESERVE  PW  3.0  NW  -1.0  ) 

SPACE  LAWYER 

TEXT  DESCRIPTION 
/*  LEGAL  COUNSEL  GIVES  POOR  PROSPECTS  OF  ANY  SIGNIFICANT  RECOVERY  FROM  THIS  DE3T0K 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE  PW  5.0    NW  -1.0    ) 

SPACE    BANXSWRSLAW 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION 
/*    3ANXROPT  AND  WRITEOFF   AND    LAWYER    •/ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

LOGICAL   DEFINITION  AND    (    3ANKSCPT  WRITEOFF   LAWYER   ) 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE   PW   9.0   NW   -2.0    ) 


-37- 


S?ACS   ECONOMIC 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION 
/*    ECONOMIC    FACTORS    ARE    CAUSING   PARTICULARLY   BAD   EFFECTS    ON  THIS    CUSTOMER'S    ABILITY 
TO   PAY    */ 

INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE   ?W    3.0    NW  0.0    ) 

SPACE   BAN&ECOSLAW* 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION 
/*   BANKRUPT   AND   ECONOMIC   AND  LAWYER   */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

LOGICAL    DEFINITION  AND    (    3ANKRUPT   ECONOMIC  LAWYER   ) 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE   PW    7.0    NW   -1.0    ) 

SPACE   GOCDRECORD 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION 
/*    THIS    DE3T0R  HAS    A  GOOD   PAST   RECORD  OF    ULTIMATELY   PAYING   SUBSTANTIALLY   ALL 
DELINQUENT  BALANCES    */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE  PW   2.0   NW   2.0    ) 

SPACE   LEGAL 

TEXT  DESCRIPTION     - 
/*   THE   MERITS   OP   THIS    DEBTOR'S    LIKELY   COUNTERCLAIM  AGAINST  YOUR  CLIENT'S    SUIT 
INDICATE    THAT  A  LEGAL  ACTION  WOULD   3E   FRUITLESS    */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES  CONSEQUENTS  (  RESERVE  PW  3.0  NW  -1.0  ) 

SPACE  LEGALSNPAY 

TEXT  DESCRIPTION 
/*  LEGAL  AND  NOT?  AY  */ 
INFERENCS 

PRIOR  0.0 

LOGICAL   DEFINITION  AND    (    LEGAL  NOT? AY   ) 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE  PW   6.0    NW  -1.0    ) 

* 

SPACE    NPAYSGDREC 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION 
/*    NOT? AY   AND   GOCDRECORD    */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

LOGICAL    DEFINITION  AND    {    NOTPAY   GOODRECORD    ) 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE   PW  -2.0   NW  0.0    ) 

STOP 


-38-- 


SPACE    NCTACTIVS 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION 
/*    NOT  OF   ACTIVE    V 

INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

logical  definition  not  active 

s?^cz  cr&np3slact 

text  description 
/*  creditmgr  and  (  not  problems   (  :;ot  active  */ 

i:jference 

PRIOR  0.0 

logical  definition  and  (  creditmgr  nctprc3lems  active  ) 
rules  consequents  (  reserve  ?w  -3.0  sw  3.0  ) 

space  correspond 

text  description 
/*  recent  correspondence  and  other  data  in  the  customer's  credit  file  supports 
3elief  in  the  c0llecta3ility  of  this  account  */ 

i:;ferencs 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE   ?W   -2.0   Nil  4.0    ) 

SPACE   CCRRES&ACT 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION 
/*   CORRESPOND  AND  ACTIVE    */' 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

LOGICAL   DEFINITION  AND    (    CORRESPOND  ACTIVE   ) 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE   P»   -3.0    KW   3.0    ) 

SPACE   CCRRESSWORJC 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION 
/*    CORRESPOND  AND  WORKOUT    */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

LOGICAL   DEFINITION  AND    (    CORRESPOND  WORKOUT   ) 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE   PW  -3.0    NW   3.0   ) 

S??JZZ  WRITEOFF 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION 

/*   TOTAL  WRITS   OFF  OF   THIS   ACCOUNT,    IF   REQOIRED,    WILL   REPRESENT  A  MATERIAL  ADJUST- 
MENT  •/ 

INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE   FW   2.0    NW  0.0    ) 

SPACE    NC RESPONSE 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION 
/*    THERE   WAS    NO    RESPONSE    TO    YOUR  CONFIRMATION    REQUEST   NCR   TO    A   FOLLCW-UP    REQUEST    */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RE^ZKTt   PW   1.0    NW   -1.0    ) 


-39- 


SPACE    COLAGENCY 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION 
/*    THIS    ACCOUNT   HAS    3EEN   ASSIGNED   TO  A   COLLECTION   AGENCY   OR  LAWYER   */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE    PW   2   NW  -1   ) 

SPACE  PORMEREMP 

TEXT  DESCRIPTION 
/♦THIS    CELHJ2UENT   ACCOUNT   IS    FROM  A  FORMER  EMPLOYEE    */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS     (    RESERVE    PW   2    NW  0    ) 

SPACE   NEW? AID 

TEXT  DESCRIPTION 
/*    DESPITE    THE    PRESENCE  OF   THIS    DELINQUENT   ITEM,    NEWER    ITEMS    HAVE    BEEN   FULLY    PAID 
INFLUENCE  .     . 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES   CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE   PW   1.5    NW  0    ) 

SPACE    NO PAY EVER 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION 
/*    NO   PAYMENTS    HAVE    EVER  3EEN   RECEIVED   FROM  THIS    CUSTOMER    */ 

PRIOR  0.0 

ROLES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE   PW   2   NW  -0.5   ) 

SPACE    ISSUENOTS 

.TEXT  DESCRIPTION 
/»    THIS    DE3T0R  HAS    ISSUED   NOTES    FOR  THE   UNPAID   PORTIONS   OF   HIS    ACCOUNT    */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES  CONSEQUENTS  (  RESERVE  PW  0.0  NW  -0.5  ) 

SPACE  OCTSAVSN? 

TEXT  DESCRIPTION 
/*  OUTSTAND  AND  AVEAGE  AND  NOT? AY  */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

LOGICAL   DEFINITION  AND    (   OUTSTAND  AVEAGE   NOT? AY   ) 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE   PW   1   NW  0   ) 

SPACE   CSTSCOSNPE 

TEXT  DESCRIPTION 
/*   CREDITSTC?   AND  COLAGENCY  AND  NCPAYEVER   */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

LOGICAL   DEFINITION  AND    (    CREDITSTCP   COLAGENCY   NCPAYEVER   ) 

^ULZS   CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE   PW   S    NW  0   ) 

SPACE   CST&NPS&WO 

PEXT  DESCRIPTION 
/*    CREDITSTC?   AND    NCPAYEVER  AND   WRITEOFF    */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

LOGICAL    DEFINITION   AND    (    CREDITSTCP    NOPAYEVER  WRITEOFF    ) 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE-  PW    3    NW  0    ) 


-40- 


SPACE  COLLECTED 

TEXT  DESCRIPTION 

/*  YOUR  TESTS  OR  CASH  RECEIPTS  INDICATE  THAT  THE  DELINQUENT  PORTIONS  OF  THE  ACCOUNT 
IN  QUESTION  HAS  3EEN  COLLECTED  SUBSTANTIALLY  IN  THEIR  ENTIRETY  SUBSEQUENT  TO  THE 
DATE  OF  THE  BALANCE  SHEET  */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES   CONSEQUENTS  (  RESERVE  PW  -30.0.  NW  1.0  ) 

SPACE  PROBLEMS 

TEXT  DESCRIPTION 
/*  SERIOUS  PROBLEMS  WITH  THE  DELINQUENT  PORTIONS  OF  THIS  ACCOUNT  VERS  REVEALED 
THROUGH  CONFIRMATION  CAUSING  YOU  TO  3ELLEVE  THEY  ARE  INVALID  */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES   CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE   PW   8.0    NW   0.0    ) 

SPACE   NOTPRCBLEMS  -     ' 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION 
/*    NOT  OF   PROBLEMS    */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

LOGICAL    DEFINITION   NOT   PROBLEMS 

SPACE   RIGOROUS 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION 
/*    COLLECTION   EFFORT   3EING  APPLIED  BY   YOUR  CLIENT   TO   COLLECT   THIS   ACCOUNT    IS   LESS 
RIGOROUS    THAN   IS    DESIRA3LE    */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES   CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE  PW   1.0    NW   0.0    ) 

SPACE  WORKOUT 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION 
/*    RECENT   COLLECTIONS    TCr'ARD   THE   DELINQUENT    PORTIONS    OF   THIS    ACCOUNT   ARE    PFOCEEDINC 
SATISFACTORILY    */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES  CONSEQUENTS  (  RESERVE  PW  -3.0  NW  3.0  ) 

SPACE  CREDITMGR 

TEXT  DESCRIPTION 
/*  THE  CREDIT  MANAGER,  OR  OTHER  COMPANY  OFFICIAL,  EXPRESSES  A  STRONG  BELIEF  IN  THE 
ULTIMATE  COLLECTION  OF  SUBSTANTIALLY  ALL  THIS  ACCOUNT  */  f 

INFERENCE  * 

PRIOR  0.0 
RULES  CONSEQUENTS  (  RESERVE  PW  -1.0  NW  2.0  ) 

SPACE  ACTIVE 

TEXT   DESCRIPTION 
/*    ALTHOUGH    A  PORTION   OF    THIS   CUSTOMER'S    TOTAL   BALANCE    IS    STILL    DELINQUENT,    HE 
CONTINUES    TO    BE    AN   ACTIVE   CUSTOMER   */ 
INFERENCE 

PRIOR  0.0 

RULES    CONSEQUENTS    (    RESERVE   PW   -2.0    NW    3.0    ) 


-41- 


REFERENCES 


Abdel-Khalik,  A.  R. ,  and  El-Sheshai ,  K.,  "Information  Choice  and  Utili- 
zation in  an  Experiment  on  Default  Prediction,"  Journal  of  Accounting 
Research  (Autumn  1980),  pp.  325-342. 

Ashton,  R.  H.,  "A  Descriptive  Study  of  Information  Evaluation,"  Journal 
of  Accounting  Research  (Spring  1981)  19. 

,  Human  Information  Processing  in  Accounting  (American  Accounting 


Association,  1982). 

Biggs,  S.  F.,  and  Mock,  T.  J.,  "An  Investigation  of  Auditor  Decision 
Processes  in  the  Evaluation  of  Internal  Controls  and  Audit  Scope 
Decisions,"  ROADS  Paper  No.  80-7,  (April  28,  1980). 

Crosby,  M.  A.,  "Bayesian  Statistics  in  Auditing:   A  Comparison  of  Prob- 
ability Elicitation  Techniques,"  Report  80-008,  Center  for  Audit 
Research,  University  of  Georgia  (1980). 

Dawes,  R.  M. ,  "The  Mind,  the  Model,  and  the  Task,"  in  F.  Restle,  R.  M. 
Shiffrin,  N.  J.  Castellan,  J.  R.  Lindman  and  D.  B.  Pisoni,  eds., 
Cognitive  Theory,  Vol.  1  (Erlbaum,  1975),  pp.  119-29. 

Duda,  R.,  Gaschnig,  J.,  and  Hart,  P.,  "Model  Design  in  the  Prospector 
Consultant  System  for  Mineral  Exploration,"  Expert  Systems  in  the 
Micro-Electronic  Age,  D.  Michie,  Ed.  (Edinburgh  University  Press, 
1979). 

Dungan,  C.  W. ,  "A  Model  of  an  Audit  Judgment  in  the  Form  of  An  Expert 
System,"  Unpubished  Ph.D.  Dissertation,  Department  of  Accountancy, 
University  of  Illinois  (1983). 

Einhorn,  H.  J.,  "Expert  Judgment:   Some  Necessary  Conditions  and  an 
Example,"  Journal  of  Applied  Psychology,  59  (1974),  pp.  562-571. 

,  Kleinmuntz,  D.  N. ,  and  Kleinmuntz,  B.,  "Linear  Regression  and 


Process-tracing  Models  of  Judgment,"  Psychological  Review,  86  (1979), 
pp.  465-485. 

Ericsson,  K.  A.,  and  Simon,  H.  A.,  "Verbal  Reports  as  Data,"  Psycholo- 
gical  Review,  Vol.  87,  No.  3,  (May  1980). 

Ernst,  G.  W. ,  and  Newell,  A.,  GPS:   A  Case  Study  in  Generality  and 


Problem  Solving  (Academic  Press,  1969). 

Feigenbaum,  E.  A.,  "The  Art  of  Artificial  Intelligence:   Themes  and  Case 
Studies  of  Knowledge  Engineering,"  Proceedings  of  the  5th  Inter- 
national  Joint  Conference  on  Artificial  Intelligence,  (1979). 


-42- 


Harrell,  A.  M. ,  "The  Decision-Making  Behavior  of  Air  Force  Officers  and 
the  Management  Control  Process,"  The  Accounting  Review,  (October 
1977),  pp.  833-841. 

Joyce,  E.  J.,  "Expert  Judgment  in  Audit  Program  Planning,"  Studies  on 
Human  Information  Processing  in  Accounting,  Supplement  to  the 
Journal  of  Accounting  Research,  (1976). 

Joyce,  E.  J.,  and  Biddle,  G.  C. ,  "Are  Auditors'  Judgments  Sufficiently 

Regressive,"  Research  Opportunities  in  Auditing  Distribution  Service, 
Peat,  Marwick,  Mitchell  Foundation,  N.Y.  #80-4,  (1980). 

Libby,  R. ,  Accounting  and  Hunan  Information  Processing  (Prentice-Hall, 
1981). 

Lichtenstein,  S.,  Fischhoff,  B.,  and  Phillips,  L.  D.,  "Calibration  of 
Probabilities:   The  State  of  the  Art,"  in  H.  Jungermann  and  G. 
de  Zeeuw  (eds.),  Decision  Making  and  Change  in  Human  Affairs, 
(Dordrecht-Holland:   Riedel,  1977),  pp.  275-324. 

Michaelsen,  R.  H.,  "A  Knowledge-Based  System  for  Individual  Income  and 
Transfer  Tax  Planning,"  Unpublished  Dissertation,  Department  of 
Accountancy,  University  of  Illinois,  (1982). 

Michie,  D.,  "Knowledge-based  Systems,"  Working  Paper  UIUCDCRS-R-80-1001 , 
Department  of  Computer  Science,  University  of  Illinois  at  Urbana- 
Champaign,  (1980). 

Newton,  L.  K. ,  "The  Risk  Factor  in  Materiality  Decisions,"  Accounting 
Review,  (January  1977)  _52_,  pp.  97-108. 

Nisbett,  R.  E.,  and  Wilson,  T.  D.,  "Telling  More  Than  We  Can  Know: 

Verbal  Reports  on  Mental  Processes,"  Psychological  Review,  Vol.  84, 
No.  3,  (May  1977),  pp.  231-259. 

Payne,  J.  W. ,  Braustein,  M.  L.,  and  Carroll,  J.  S. ,  "Exploring  Pre- 

Decisional  Behavior:   An  Alternative  Approach  to  Decision  Research," 
Organizational  Behavior  and  Human  Performance,  (February  1978)  22, 
pp.  17-44. 

Reiter,  J.,  AL/X:   An  Expert  System  Using  Plausible  Inference,  (Intel- 
ligent Terminals  Ltd.,  1980). 

Reitman,  W.  R. ,  Cognition  and  Thought,  (Wiley,  1965). 

Shields,  M.  D.,  "Some  Effects  of  Information  Load  on  Search  Patterns 
Used  to  Analyze  Performance  Reports,"  Unpublished  manuscript, 
University  of  North  Carolina,  Chapel  Hill,  (May  1980). 

Shortliffe,  E.  H.,  And  Buchanan,  B.  G. ,  "A  Model  of  Inexact  Reasoning 

in  Medicine,"  Mathematical  Bioscience,  Vol.  23,  (1975),  pp.  351-379. 


-43- 


Simon,  H.  A.,  "On  How  to  Decide  What  To  Do,"  Bell  Journal  of  Economics, 
(Autumn  1978),  pp.  494-507. 

Turing,  A.  M. ,  "Computing  Machinery  and  Intelligence,"  Mind,  (October 

1950),  reprint  in  Creative  Computing,  Vol.  6,  No.  1,  (January  1980), 
pp.  44-53. 

Yu,  V.  L.,  Fagan,  L.  M. ,  Wraith,  S.  et .  al.,  "Anti-microbial  Selection 
by  a  Computer:   A  Blinded  Evaluation  by  Infectious  Diseases  Experts, 
Journal  of  the  American  Medical  Association,  Vol.  242,  No.  12, 
(September  21,  1979),  pp.  1279-1282. 

Zadeh,  L.,  "A  Theory  of  Approximate  Reasoning,"  in  Machine  Intelligence, 
_9_,  J.  R.  Hayes,  D.  Michie,  and  L.  Mikulich  (Eds.),  (Ellis  Norwood 
Ltd.,  and  John  Wiley  and  Sons,  1979). 


D/174