Skip to main content

Full text of "Apple pollination studies in the Annapolis Valley, N.S., Canada, 1928-1932."

See other formats


V 


•'>'>\pv  i>r»>"»T 


7  VMsit 


APPLE  POLLINATION  STUDIES 


IN  THE 


ANNAPOLIS  VALLEY,  N.S. 

CANADA 


1928-1932 


UNDER  THE  DIRECTION  OF 

W.  H.  BRITTAIN 

PROPERTY  OF  LIBRARY 
DEPARTMENT  OF  AGRICULTURE,  OTTAWA 

AL32    25169-ISM-5fea^ 


PLEASE  RETURN 


DOMINION  OF  CANADA 

DEPARTMENT  OF  AGRICULTURE 


BULLETIN  No.  162— NEW  SERIES 


630.4 
C212 

B   /£2 

new  ser. 


Published  by  direction  of  the  Hon.  Robert  Weir,  Minister  of  Agriculture. 
May,  1933 


> 


iilllllMlllill 


CONTENTS 


Page 

Popular  Summary 5 

Foreword 11 

Acknowledgments  and  List  of  Workers 12 

I.  Introduction 13 

(a)  The  Apple  Industry  in  Nova  Scotia 13 

(b)  Factors  Other  Than  Pollination  Affecting  Fruit  Production 18 

II.  Pollination  and  Fruitfulness  in  Apples.  . 24 

(a)  The  Process  of  Pollination  and  Fertilization  (Definitions) 24 

(b)  The  Pollination  Problem 25 

(c)  Causes  of  Unfruitfulness 26 

(d)  Percentage  of  Fruit  to  Flowers  Required  to  Give  an  Economic  Yield 29 

III.  Experimental  Studies  in  Apple  Pollination 31 

(a)  Conditions  for  Cross-Pollination 31 

(b)  Previous  Work  with  Bees  as  Pollinators  under  Controlled  Conditions 31 

(c)  Experiments  in  Bees  and  Pollination  (Tent  Studies) 32 

(d)  Studies  in  the  Inter-fruitfulness  of  Standard  Varieties 37 

(e)  Pollination  Tests  with  Blenheim  and  Stark 74 

(f)  Planning  the  Orchard 82 

(g)  Relation  of  Fruit  Set  on  Entire  Tree  to  that  on  Individual  Limbs  or  Spurs 86 

(h)  Over-pollination 86 

(i)    Temporary  Provision  of  Pollen 87 

(j)   Inhibiting  Effect  of  Unfruitful  Pollen 89 

(k)  Variation  in  Self-Fruitfulness 90 

IV.  Field  Studies  in  the  Use  of  Insects  as  Orchard^Pollinators 91 

(a)  Introduction 91 

(b)  General : : 91 

(c)  Insects  Concerned 91 

(d)  Relative  Value  of  Insect  Pollinators 100 

(e)  Utilization  of  Hive  Bees  as  Orchard  Pollinators 134 

V.  Studies  in  Bee  Poisoning  as  a  Phase  of  Orchard  Pollination  Studies 158 

(a)  Introduction 158 

(b)  Historical 158 

(c)  Development  of  Problem 161 

(d)  Experiments  in  Bee  Poisoning 163 

VI.  Literature  Cited 190 

Index 195 


60796-l£ 


Digitized  by  the  Internet  Archive 
in  2013 


http://archive.org/details/applepollination162brit 


ERRATA 

Page  5,  line  23,  for  "  anthers  discharge  their  pollen  shortly  before  the  stigmas 
are  receptive  "  read  the  stigmas  become  receptive  shortly  before  the 
anthers  dehisce. 

Page  8,  line  19,  for  "  apple  "  read  pollen,  and  in  line  26,  for  "  later"  read  earlier 
and  for  "  earlier  "  read  later 

Page  12,  line  23,  for  "  C.  A.  Ativood  "  read  C.  E.  Atwood. 

Page  13,  linel$h  for  "  south  "  read  north. 

Page  14,  fig.  2,  for  "  7  mile— I  inch  "  read   -26"=1  mile. 

Page  19,  line  39,  for  "  7332"  read  J0SJ. 

Page  29,  line  22,  for  "  in  the  case  "  read  is  the  case. 

Page  44,  line  16,  for  "  aberation  "  read  aberration. 

Page  46.  fig.  11,  for  top  percentage  figure  "  85  "  read  90. 

Page  48,  line  3,  for  "  varied  "  read  varies. 

Page  50,  line  5  and  6,  for  "  as  in  the  case  of  the  "  read  due  to  certain. 

Page  86.  line  21,  for  "  each  year/'  read  each  alternate  year. 

Page  102,  fig.  40,  for  "  Scale  4"—l  m^e  "  read  scale  -66"=1  mile. 

Page  104,  fig.  42,  for  u  4"  =  1  mile"  read  -55"— 1  mile. 

Page  104,  fig.  43,  for  "Scale  4"—l  mile"  read  scale   -oo"  =  l  mile. 

Page  108,  line  6,  for  "  over  winter"  read  overwinter  and  in  line  33,  for  "  1928- 
31  "  read  1928-1930. 

Page  116,  line  1,  for  "  5300A  "  read  5300A  and  in  line  3.  "  5530A  "  read  5530A. 

Page  120,  line  4,  for  "  falls  "  read  falling. 

Page  129,  line  1,  for  "  22."  read  22y 

Page  144,  line  47,  for  "  result  "  read  results. 

Page  153,  line  20,  for  "  1.23"  read  1.94,  and  in  line  40,  for  "  1.27"  read  1.94. 

Page  155,  line  26,  omit  comma  after  "  may." 

Page  159,  line  30,  for  "  the  bees  were  found  to  6c",  read  it  was  found  that  thi 
bees  wen  . 

Page  160,  line   18,  for  "  data  "  read  date. 

Page  167,  line  14,  for  "  e.g."  read  i.e. 

Page  168,  line  6,  for  "  sprat/   dust  "  read  spray  and  dust  and  in  line  33,  for 
"  arsenical  and  "  read  arsenicals  and. 

Page  173,  heading  table  No.  34,  for  "  1920"  read  1929. 

Page  175,  last  line,  for  "  1 928-1931  "  rend  1928  and  1929. 

Page   176,  line  29,  for  "orchard"  read  orchard  being  available. 

Page  188,  line  39,  for  "  .0008"  read  .00008. 

Page  190,  under  Darlington,  C.  D.,  and  Moffatt,  A.  A.,  delete  1926  citation. 


APPLE  POLLINATION  STUDIES  IN  THE 
ANNAPOLIS  VALLEY 

BY   W.    H.    BRITTAIN 


POPULAR   SUMMARY 

GENERAL 

The  investigations  described  in  the  following  pages  grew  out  of  a  request 
of  the  Department  of  Agriculture  of  Nova  Scotia  and  of  various  horticultural 
organizations,  that  a  study  be  undertaken  to  determine  whether  the  alleged 
destruction  of  pollinating  insects  by  poison  dusts  and  sprays  was  adversely  affect- 
ing the  set  of  fruit  in  commercial  apple  orchards. 

As  a  result  of  the  initial  survey,  the  work  quickly  broadened  out  into  a 
study  of  the  entire  pollination  problem.  As  indicated  in  the  evidence  presented, 
it  was  found  that  the  hive  bees  of  the  Annapolis  valley  had  indeed  suffered 
great  losses  as  a  result  of  prevailing  spraying  and  dusting  practices.  However, 
it  was  found  that  wild  solitary  bees  were  sufficiently  abundant  in  most  orchards 
during  the  period  of  the  investigation,  alone  to  effect  pollination  under  favour- 
able conditions.  The  results  secured  emphasized  that  the  proper  interplanting 
of  cross-fruitful  varieties  was  a  crucial  factor  in  the  production  of  commercial 
crops. 

THE    PROCESS    OF    POLLINATION    AND    FERTILIZATION    IN    APPLES 

The  apple  blossom  is  provided  with  five  sepals,  which  persist  at  the  "  blos- 
som end  "  of  the  apple,  five  petals,  which  are  soon  shed,  twenty  to  twenty-five 
stamens,  each  with  an  anther  containing  pollen  surmounting  the  filament  and 
surrounding  the  five  stigmas,  which  unite  in  a  common  style  that  leads  to  the 
ovary,  these  parts  constituting  the  pistil  of  the  flower.  The  stamens  and  pistil 
are  the  essential  organs  of  the  flower  representing,  respectively,  the  male  and 
female  parts. 

The  ovary  is  divided  into  five  compartments  each  containing  two  egg  cells, 
or  four  in  the  case  of  Northern  Spy.  As  the  anthers  discharge  their  pollen  shortly 
before  the  stigmas  are  receptive,  this  renders  it  difficult  for  pollen  from  a  flower 
to  reach  the  stigmas  of  the  same  flower.  There  may  be,  however,  a  considerable 
number  of  blossoms  on  the  tree  that  receive  pollen  from  their  own  or  neighbour- 
ing flowers. 

The  mere  transfer  of  the  pollen  from  anther  to  stigma  constitutes  pollination 
and  is  usually  effected  by  the  aid  of  insects.  The  transfer  of  pollen  from  the 
anther  of  one  flower  to  the  stigma  of  another  of  the  same  variety  is  known 
as  self-pollination.  The  transfer  to  the  stigma  of  the  flower  of  another  variety 
is  cross-pollination.  Following  pollination  with  the  pollen  of  a  suitable  variety, 
the  pollen  tube  develops,  grows  down  the  style  through  the  tissue  and  finally 
reaches  the  ovary,  where,  upon  penetration,  the  sperm  is  discharged  into  the 
embryo  sac,  where  it  unites  with  the  egg  cell,  thus  accomplishing  fertilization, 
this  process  usually  resulting  in  the  formation  of  seed.  This  initiates  the  growth 
and  development  of  the  fruit  and  is  requisite  to  setting.  Where  the  ovules  fail 
of  fertilization,  or  where,  for  any  reason  development  is  checked  the  blossoms 
are  soon  shed. 

It  is  not,  of  course,  necessary  for  the  entire  complement  of  seeds  to  be  pro- 
duced in  order  for  normal  apples  to  develop,  but  the  larger  the  number  of  seeds 


that  do  develop  in  a  fruit  the  better  chance  is  there  for  that  fruit  to  succeed  in 
the  competition  for  nutrients  and  to  remain  on  the  tree  until  the  harvest.  Apples 
with  one  or  more  compartments  devoid  of  seed  are  likely  to  be  one  sided  or 
otherwise  abnormal.  While  it  is  true  that  a  certain  amount  of  fruit  may  develop 
without  seed  in  some  varieties,  especially  in  Gravenstein,  this  has  little  commer- 
cial significance  as  the  percentage  so  produced  is  small. 

From  the  standpoint  of  their  requirements  for  pollination  apples  may  be 
either  (1)  self -fruitful  or  (2)  self -unfruitful,  i.e.,  they  are  capable  of  producing 
mature  fruit  when  pollinated  with  their  own  pollen  or  (2)  they  require  the 
pollen  from  another  variety  in  order  to  produce  fruit.  We  shall  see  later  that 
few,  if  any,  varieties  are  completely  self-unfruitful,  and  hence  the  expression 
"  partially  self-fruitful  "  largely  loses  its  meaning.  In  fact,  self-fruitfulness  varies 
from  varieties  that  yield  little  or  no  fruit  when  self-pollinated  to  others  that 
produce  a  set  little  short  of  that  obtained  from  favourable  cross-pollinations. 

When  the  pollen  from  one  variety  results  in  fruit  production  when  placed 
on  the  stigma  of  another  variety,  the  first  variety  is  said  to  be  cross-fruitful 
with  the  second  variety.  Just  as  few  apple  varieties  are  completely  self-unfruit- 
ful, so  there  are  few  cases  in  which  the  pollen  from  one  variety  is  completely 
useless  for  another  variety,  though  there  are  many  combinations  that  give  very 
poor  results  commercially.  Therefore,  the  expressions  "  commercially  self-  or 
cross-fruitful  "  would  be  more  accurate. 

THE   PROBLEM   OF   POLLINATION 

In  order  for  apple  orchards  to  be  pollinated  there  must  be  present  in  the 
orchard  (1)  a  suitable  pollen  supply  in  the  form  of  varieties  capable  of  pro- 
ducing fruit  when  the  pollen  is  carried  to  the  stigmas  of  other  varieties  present 
and  (2)  an  adequate  supply  of  insect  pollinators  in  the  form  of  either  hive  or 
wild  bees,  to  ensure  that  cross-pollination  occurs.  This  is  because  all  varieties 
of  apples  produce  better  crops  when  cross-pollinated  by  another  suitable  variety 
and  some  of  them  ordinarily  set  very  little  when  self-pollinated.  The  admix- 
ture of  varieties  is  important,  because  some  varieties  yield  pollen  which,  when 
appli  d  to  the  stigmas  of  certain  others,  results  in  low  yields,  sometimes  even  less 
than  when  the  same  variety  is  self-pollinated.  All  varieties  require  the  work  of 
insect  pollinators,  even  the  most  self-fruitful,  since  wind  is  a  negligible  factor 
in  the  pollination  of  the  apple. 

The  problem  of  apple  pollination  is  rendered  much  less  acute  than  it  might 
otherwise  be  by  the  fact  that  only  a  small  amount  of  pollination  is  necessary.  If 
one  out  of  twenty  of  the  original  blossoms  develops  into  fruit  a  commercial  crop 
will  result,  provided  there  is  a  good  bloom,  whereas,  in  the  cherry,  for  example,  a 
much  larger  set  is  required.  Again,  self-  and  cross-unfruitfulness  are  much  less 
pronounced  in  the  apple  than  in  the  cherry.  It  may  also  be  noted  that  since  the 
cherry  has  but  a  single  seed,  if  that  seed  fails  to  be  fertilized  the  fruit  drops. 
Whereas,  in  the  apple  with  its  complement  of  ten  or  twenty  seeds,  the  fertilization 
of  but  a  small  proportion  is  necessary  for  setting  and  even  seniles  apples  may 
be  produced  by  some  varieties. 

In  one  variety  studied,  viz.,  the  Baldwin,  selfing  may  resull  in  a  commercial 
crop,  though  not  as  high  a  yield  as  is  produced  from  the  most  fruitful  crosses.  In 
view  of  these  facts,  it  is  only  to  be  expected  that  less  difficulty  would  be  experi- 
enced in  ensuring  cross-pollination  of  apple  varieties  than  would  occur  in  the 
cherry,  where  pronounced  self-unfruitfulness  and  cross-unfruitfulness  may  occur. 
On  the  other  hand,  cases  of  over-pollination,  resulting  in  biennial  bearing,  appear 
to  occur  in  some  varieties.  Nevertheless  it  should  be  emphasized  that,  with  the 
possible  exception  of  Baldwin  all  varieties  must  be  regarded  as  commercially 
self-unfruitful  and  that  all,  including  Baldwin,  benefit  from  cross-pollination.  A 
definite  problem  results  when  (1)  large  blocks  of  highly  self-unfruitful  varieties 


are  planted  together  or  (2)  when  cross-unfruitful  varieties  are  mixed  or  (3) 
when  insect  pollinators  are  insufficient  in  number.  The  question  of  the  inter- 
fruitfulness  of  varieties  should  be  given  consideration  in  connection  with  all 
grafting-out  operations  and  also  when  new  plantings  of  commercially  desirable 
sorts  are  to  be  made. 

To  elaborate  further  upon  this  point  it  may  be  said  that  with  respect  to 
the  two  fundamentals  for  successful  pollination,  viz.,  (1)  pollen  supply  and 
(2)  insect  pollinators,  the  following  conditions  may  exist  in  commercial  orchards: 

1.  There  may  be  a  suitable  intermixture  of  inter-fruitful  varieties  and  an 
adequate  population  of  insect  pollinators. 

2.  Cross-unfruitful  varieties  may  be  inter-planted,  though  insect  pollinators 
are  sufficiently  abundant. 

3.  Varieties  may  be  planted  together  in  too  large  blocks  to  permit  of  effect- 
ive cross-pollination,  even  though  an  abundance  of  insect  pollinators  may  be 
present. 

4.  A  similar  condition  to  number  3,  but  without  a  sufficient  number  of  insect 
pollinators. 

5.  Similar  to  number  1,  but  with  an  inadequate  number  of  insect  pollinators. 

To  simulate  under  experimental  control,  the  foregoing  conditions,  trees  of 
the  Gravenstein,  King,  Baldwin,  and  Spy  varieties  were  covered  with  tents  and 
the  following  treatments  given: — 

1.  Supplied  with  "  bouquets,"  i.e.,  blossoming  limbs  of  the  desired  variety 

placed  iri  tubs  of  water,  of  an  effective  pollinizer  and  a  hive  of  bees. 

2.  Supplied  with   "  bouquets  "   of   an   ineffective   pollinizer  and   a   hive   of 

bees. 

3.  Not  supplied  with  "  bouquets  "  of  any  kind  but  with  a  hive  of  bees. 

4.  Not  supplied  with  "  bouquets  "  of  any  kind  nor  with  a  hive  of  bees. 

5.  Supplied  with  "  bouquets  "  of  an  effective  pollinizer  but  no  bees. 

6.  Un-tented  trees  left  to  open  pollination  for  comparison. 

TABLE  No.  1.— RESULTS  OF  TENT  EXPERIMENTS,  1929-1932 


Variety 

Effective 
pollinizer 
and  bees 

Ineffec- 
tive 
pollinizer 
and  bees 

No 

pollinizer 

and  bees 

(selfed) 

No 
pollinizer 

and 
no  bees 

Effective 
pollinizer 

but 
no  bees 

Open 
polli- 
nation 

Gravenstein 

King 

Baldwin 

Spy 

10-90 
5-42 
8-17 

10  05 

1-14 
3-58 
4-96 
2-70 

2-12 
3-32 

7-77 
200 

0-67 
103 
3-49 
0-85 

0-47 
1-81 
5-05* 
1-20 

9-91 
4-74 
10-40 

8-83 

*Abnormally  high  per  cent  set  in  1932  due  to  small  number  of  blossoms  on  tree,  has  raised  the  general 
average  for  this  treatment. 

The  results  indicated  in  the  accompanying  table  summarizing  four  years 
work,  justify  the  following  conclusions:  — 

1.  Insect  pollinators  are  required  by  all  varieties,  wind  pollination  alone 
giving  unsatisfactory  results. 

2.  All  varieties  give  best  results  when  crossed  with  an  effective  pollinizer. 

3.  Only  the  Baldwin  yields  a  commercial  crop  when  self-pollinated  and 
even  the  Baldwin  gives  better  results  when  crossed  with  an  effective  pollinizer. 

STUDIES  IN   INTER-FRUITFULNESS   OF  APPLE   VARIETIES 

During  the  past  four  years  very  many  extensive  crosses  and  self-pollina- 
tions have  been  made  with  standard  varieties  grown  in  Nova  Scotia  and  much 
information  with  respect  to  the  value  of  varieties  from  the  standpoint  of  self- 
fruitfulness  and  of  cross-fruitfulness  have  been  obtained.     It  is  not  necessary 


8 

to  detail  the  results  of  these  experiments,  but  rather  to  point  out  the  practical 
significance  to  the  grower  of  the  results  secured. 

A  point  of  prime  importance  brought  out  in  these  studies  was  that  from 
the  standpoint  of  their  value  as  pollen  parents,  i.e.,  as  pollinizers  for  other 
varieties,  apple  varieties  may  be  sharply  divided  into  two  groups,  namely,  (1) 
good  pollen  varieties  and  (2)  poor  pollen  varieties.  The  former  may  be  usually 
depended  upon  to  work  well  as  pollinizers  with  other  varieties  that  blossom  at 
the  same  time.  The  latter  ordinarily  have  little  value  as  pollen  parents,  some 
of  them  producing  in  the  variety  pollinated  even  less  fruit  than  when  the  variety 
on  which  this  pollen  is  used  is  self-pollinated.  Poor  pollen  producers  may 
themselves  be  highly  fruitful,  when  pollinated  by  one  of  the  group  of  good  pollen 
producers.  The  poor  pollen  producers  are  known  not  only  by  the  small  quan- 
tity of  pollen  produced  of  low  germinability  and  inferior  value  for  crossing 
purposes,  but  by  their  relatively  low  seed  content  as  compared  with  good  pollen 
producers.  This  point  is  of  the  utmost  importance  to  the  grower  who  has  a 
pollination  problem  in  his  orchard  or  who  desires  to  make  new  plantings.  The 
practical  results  of  our  studies  can  best  be  appreciated  by  a  study  of  the  follow- 
ing table  in  which  some  of  the  common  varieties  are  listed  as  good  or  poor 
apple  producers.  By  consulting  the  blossoming  chart  in  connection  therewith, 
also  the  chart  showing  the  inter-fruitfulness  of  some  standard  varieties  and  a 
third  chart  showing  the  results  of  hand-pollination  experiments,  together  with 
the  accompanying  table  of  good  and  poor  pollen  producers,  the  grower  can 
decide  which  varieties  "  go  well  "  together  and  avoid  those  combinations  that 
are  sure  to  give  inferior  results.  In  providing  pollinizers  for  varieties  that  have 
given  poor  results  due  to  lack  of  pollination,  it  is  preferable  to  choose  a  variety 
that  blossoms  a  little  later  rather  than  a  little  earlier,  because,  in  the  first  case, 
the  pollen  of  the  pollinizing  variety  will  be  liberated  before  that  of  the  other 
is  shed,  thus  facilitating  cross-pollination.  The  blossoming  chart  shows  only 
average  blooming  periods,  but,  in  some  seasons,  there  is  much  less  overlapping 
than  others,  and  the  more  long  drawn  out  the  bloom  the  greater  likelihood  of  a 
pollination  problem  arising,  unless  adequate  provision  has  been  made. 

TABLE  No.  2.— CLASSIFICATION  OF  APPLE  VARIETIES 


Poor  pollen  producers 

( rood  pollen  producers 

Baldwin 

Alexander 

Blenheim 

Ben  Davis 

Bramley  Seedling 

Hough  Sweet 

Gravenstein 

( Portland 

King 

(  Ox  Orange 

Mann 

Crimson  Beauty 

Nonpareil  (Roxbury  Russet) 

Duchess 

Ribston 

Dudley 

Rhode  Island  Greening 

Delicious 

Stark 

Fameuse 

Gano 

( rolden  Russel 

(J rimes  G olden 

Jonathan 

Mcintosh 

Rome  Beauty 

Tolman  Sweet 

Wagener 

Wealthy 

Wellington 

Winter  Banana 

Wolf  River 

Yellow  Bellflower 

Yellow  Transparent 

York  Imperial 

FIELD    STUDIES   IN   INSECT   POLLINATORS 

As  already  clearly  shown  in  the  foregoing  experiments,  apple  pollination 
is  effected  by  insects,  and  it  is  usually  claimed  that  the  hive  bee  is  the  main 
agent  in  apple  pollination.  Unfortunately,  our  studies  show  that  as  a  result  of 
widespread  poisoning  from  the  use  of  poisoned  sprays  and  dusts,  the  hive  bee 
has  ceased  to  be  a  factor  in  apple  pollination  in  the  Annapolis  valley  of  Nova 
Scotia.  The  danger  of  poisoning  hive  bees  may  be  reduced,  though  not  entirely 
eliminated,  by  refraining  from  spraying  and  dusting  during  the  blossoming 
period  and  by  moving  the  bees  into  the  orchard  only  when  the  early  apples 
have  come  into  bloom  and  removing  them  before  the  after-blossom  sprays  are 
applied.  Dusting  is  usually  more  fatal  than  spraying,  but  severe  losses  may 
follow  either  practice  when  poisons  are  applied  to  apple  bloom,  or  to  the  blossoms 
of  other  plants  growing  in  or  near  the  orchard. 

Careful  investigations  have  shown  that  pollination  at  present  is  mainly 
effected  by  various  small,  solitary  bees  that  nest  in  the  ground  in  the  neighbour- 
hood of  orchards,  especially  along  roadsides,  pastures,  dykes  and  similar  situa- 
tions. They  have  been  reasonably  plentiful  in  at  least  most  orchards  during  the 
years  1928  to  1931  inclusive.  In  1932,  there  was  an  apparent  general  decrease 
and  this,  combined  with  unfavourable  weather  for  bee  activity  during  the 
blossoming  period,  had  a  noticeable  effect  on  fruit  setting.  Bumble  bees,  while 
sometimes  abundant  locally,  were  of  minor  importance  in  most  orchards  during 
the  course  of  our  studies. 

While,  therefore,  under  favourable  conditions,  solitary  bees  may  alone  be 
able  to  satisfactorily  pollinate  the  apple  crop,  conditions  may  arise  when  it  is 
desirable  to  supplement  their  efforts.  This  can  only  be  done  by  supplying  hive 
bees  for  the  purpose. 

It  should  be  emphasized,  however,  that  a  few  colonies  of  bees  placed  in  an 
orchard  surrounded  by  large  acreages  devoid  of  bees  is  of  little  or  no  value.  In 
such  situations  it  may  be  necessary  to  have  a  concentration  of  from  35  to  50 
colonies  in  order  to  ensure  the  pollination  of  the  particular  orchard  in  which 
the  bees  are  placed.  In  districts  where  beekeeping  is  general,  however,  and 
neighbouring  orchards  are  similarly  supplied,  one  colony  to  the  acre  or  even 
one  colony  to  four  acres  may  be  sufficient.  Owing  to  the  many  factors  involved 
more  exact  figures  cannot  be  given.  It  must  suffice  to  point  out  that  the  pro- 
vision of  as  many  colonies  as  practicable  is  a  useful  measure  of  insurance  against 
unfavourable  weather,  and  a  scarcity  of  wild  pollinators,  since  it  is  only  the  hive 
bees  that  can  be  increased  in  numbers  at  will  and  placed  where  needed  in  the 
orchards.  Unfortunately,  at  the  present  time,  there  is  no  adequate  local  supply; 
inexperience  in  beekeeping  and  the  danger  of  poisoning  prevents  many  from 
adopting  this  practice  who  would  otherwise  do  so. 

ARRANGEMENT   OF   POLLINIZERS 

In  orchard  planning,  the  mixture  of  self-unfruitful  varieties  should  be 
avoided,  and  this  includes  practically  all  varieties,  save  Baldwin.  A  great  deal 
of  trouble  is  experienced  with  Blenheims  planted  in  solid  blocks.  Not  all  Blen- 
heims even  in  mixed  orchards  are  fruitful,  but  more  of  them  are,  whereas  we 
do  not  know  of  large  unmixed  blocks  bearing  regular  crops.  The  same  may 
be  said  of  Starks.  Large  blocks  of  a  single  variety  are  uncommon  in  Nova 
Scotia  and  this  has  greatly  reduced  the  trouble  due  to  lack  of  pollination,  but 
lack  of  bearing  due  to  too  many  trees  in  a  block  has  been  noted,  not  only  in 
the  foregoing  varieties,  but  also,  though  less  commonly,  in  certain  other  varie- 
ties, including  Golden  Russet,  Spy  and  Gravenstein. 

Bad  combinations  of  cross-unfruitful  sorts  are  unfortunately  more  common 
and  are  especially  to  be  avoided.  Thus  a  combination  of  Blenheim  and  Baldwin 
is  bad,  because  though  the  Baldwin  may  give  fair  crops  in  its  bearing  years, 


10 

though  probably  less  than  in  a  block  by  itself,  the  Blenheim  is  entirely  without 
a  suitable  supply  of  pollen,  and,  being  very  self-unfruitful,  is  likely  to  give  very 
inferior  results.  Stark  and  Baldwin  or  Stark  and  Blenheim  is  another  bad 
combination  and,  worse  still,  a  combination  of  all  three.  With  King  the  pollen 
variety  seems  to  make  less  difference  than  with  most  others.  This  variety 
typically  producing  a  low  percentage  of  fruit,  no  matter  what  pollen  parent 
is  used,  nevertheless  is  commercially  fruitful  because  of  its  heavy  blossoming 
habit,  large  size  and  annual  bearing  habit.  However,  even  King  responds  by 
increased  yields  to  an  effective  pollinizer,  but  is  itself  of  little  use  as  a  pollen 
parent.  Therefore,  where  King  occurs  in  planting,  a  suitable  pollinizer  must 
be  provided  and  the  same  may  be  said  of  Gravenstein  and  other  poor  pollen 
producers. 

Furthermore,  it  must  be  borne  in  mind  that,  when  supplying  a  pollinizer 
for  a  variety  that  itself  is  a  poor  pollen  producer,  a  third  variety  must  be  used 
that  is  cross-fruitful  with  the  first  pollinizer. 

The  accompanying  plans  showing  method  of  setting  out  a  four-variety 
block  and  for  grafting-out  a  solid  block  of  one  variety  will  be  suggestive  of 
many  similar  combinations  that  may  be  used. 

OTHER  CAUSES  OF  UNFRUITFULNESS 

It  should  be  emphasized  that  lack  of  pollination  is  but  one  of  the  causes 
of  unfruitfulness ;  lack  of  adequate  attention  to  fertilizer  requirements  and 
damage  caused  by  insect  or  fungous  pests  being  of  most  importance.  Further- 
more, vigorous  fruit-spurs  are  more  likely  to  set  fruit  with  less  pollination  than 
weak  spurs.  Pollination  alone,  therefore,  cannot  ensure  regular  cropping,  which 
can  only  be  effected  by  attention  to  all  the  necessary  details  of  good  orchard 
practice. 


APPLE  POLLINATION  STUDIES  IN  THE 
ANNAPOLIS  VALLEY 


FOREWORD 

The  investigations  described  in  the  following  pages  were  undertaken  as  a 
result  of  resolutions  received  by  the  Honourable  the  Minister  of  Agriculture, 
from  the  Canadian  Horticultural  Council,  the  Nova  Scotia  Fruit  Growers'  Asso- 
ciation and  other  organizations,  requesting  that  an  investigation  be  undertaken 
into  the  whole  pollination  problem  in  Nova  Scotia  and  whether  the  alleged 
destruction  of  pollinating  insects  as  a  result  of  poison  dusts  had  adversely 
influenced  the  setting  of  fruit  during  the  past  few  years.  Upon  receipt  of  these 
resolutions  a  committee  was  appointed  by  the  Deputy  Minister  to  consider  and 
report  upon  the  advisability  of  initiating  such  an  investigation.  This  committee 
was  as  follows: 

Arthur  Gibson,  Dominion  Entomologist   (Chairman) 
W.  T.  Macoun,  Dominion  Horticulturist 
C.  B.  Gooderham,  Dominion  Apiarist  (Secretary) 

H.  G.  Crawford,  Chief,  Division  of  Field  Crop  and  Garden  Insects,  Ento- 
mological Branch 
F.  A.  Herman,  Division  of  Chemistry,  Central  Experimental  Farm 
H.  Groh,  Division  of  Botany,  Central  Experimental  Farm 

As  a  result  of  the  deliberations  of  this  committee  a  report  was  prepared  for 
the  Deputy  Minister  approving  the  carrying  out  of  the  proposed  investigation, 
but  recognizing  that  factors  affecting  the  set  of  fruit  other  than  insects, 
would  have  to  be  taken  into  consideration  in  the  course  of  such  studies,  they 
expressed  the  opinion  that  such  an  investigation  could  not  be  completed  in  less 
time  than  five  years  and  drew  up  a  tentative  scheme  for  the  conduct  of  the 
experiments.  They  further  recommended  that  Dr.  W.  H.  Brittain,  Professor  of 
Entomology,  Macdonald  College,  be  asked  to  take  over  the  general  supervision 
of  the  experiments,  working  in  co-operation  with  the  officials  of  the  Dominion 
Department  of  Agriculture,  whose  work  had  a  direct  bearing  on  the  problem 
involved.  It  was  understood  that  the  problem  would  not  come  under  any  one 
Branch,  but  that  the  officer  in  charge  would  attempt  to  co-ordinate  the  work  of 
the  different  agencies  concerned.  The  Department  of  Agriculture  for  Nova 
Scotia  took  an  active  interest  in  planning  the  work,  and  the  Deputy  Minister, 
Col.  R.  Innes,  placed  an  assistant  and  office  facilities  at  the  disposal  of  the  com- 
mittee. 

The  foregoing  plan  having  received  the  approval  of  the  Deputy  Minister 
and  the  sanction  of  the  Minister,  was  proceeded  with  and  an  effort  made  to  devote 
as  much  attention  as  possible  to  the  significant  factors  involved  in  apple  pollina- 
tion. The  study  falls  naturally  into  two  parts:  first,  a  study  of  the  inter-fruit- 
fulness  of  some  of  the  chief  commercial  varieties  grown  in  Nova  Scotia  and, 
second,  a  study  of  the  insects  involved  in  the  pollinizing  process.  While  the  study 
dealt  primarily  with  local  varieties  and  problems  an  attempt  was  made  to  pay 
particular  attention  to  a  study  of  factors  and  principles  applicable  to  all  work 
of  this  sort. 

11 


12 

Considerations  of  space  have  forbidden  the  publication  of  a  great  deal  of 
data  secured  during  the  course  of  these  investigations,  some  of  which  have  an 
important  bearing  on  the  work.  For  this  reason  much  tabular  matter,  weather 
records,  results  of  analyses  and  other  matter  has  had  to  be  excluded,  while 
detailed  accounts  of  other  phases  of  the  work  will  be  published  in  separate 
papers. 

The  five-year  period  has  proved  sufficient  to  elucidate  most  of  the  problems 
dealing  with  the  inter-fruitfulness  of  the  apple  varieties  studied,  and  to  throw 
some  light  upon  the  general  problem  of  apple  pollination.  A  number  of  points 
of  considerable  theoretical  importance,  as  indicated  in  the  following  pages,  still 
awaits  attention.  It  is  to  be  regretted  that  lack  of  time,  the  necessity  of  develop- 
ing a  suitable  technique,  want  of  suitable  equipment,  poisoning  of  our  experi- 
mental apiaries  and  other  causes  have  not  permitted  us  to  bring  the  entomological 
phases  of  the  investigation  to  the  same  degree  of  completeness.  Consequently 
many  of  the  conclusions  must  be  regarded  as  tentative. 

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS 

Grateful  acknowledgment  is  due  from  the  officer  in  charge  of  these  investiga- 
tions to  colleagues  and  assistants  who  have  helped  him  in  securing  the  data 
contained  herein.  The  names  of  several  appear  as  joint  authors  of  sections  of 
this  report,  while  the  assistance  of  others  is  acknowledged  elsewhere. 

Mr.  F.  A.  Herman  was  associated  in  the  poisoning  investigations;  Mr.  C.  B. 
Gooderham  in  the  utilization  of  hive  bees  in  orchards;  Mr.  Don  Blair  in  studies 
of  inter-fruitfulness  of  apple  varieties;  Mr.  J.  M.  Cameron  in  studies  of  the 
activities  of  hive  and  wild  bees  and  Mr.  C.  A.  At  wood  in  studies  of  the  biology 
and  classification  of  insect  pollinators. 

At  the  outset  of  the  investigation  it  was  necessary  to  proceed  with  impro- 
vised methods  and  a  temporary  staff  and  through  the  course  of  the  work  this 
occasioned  many  difficulties.  Nevertheless,  the  smoothness  with  which  the  work 
proceeded  after  the  first  year,  speaks  very  well  indeed  for  the  industry  and 
initiative  of  the  various  workers.  It  would  be  difficult  to  imagine  a  temporary 
organization  working  together  more  smoothly  in  carrying  out  the  work  for 
which  each  was  responsible  in  connection  with  this   complicated  problem. 

In  addition  to  those  who  have  been  solely  or  jointly  responsible  for  definite 
parts  of  the  investigation,  it  would  be  impossible  to  mention  by  name  all  those 
whose  efforts  have  been  placed  freely  at  our  disposal  in  the  course  of  these 
studies.  Dr.  W.  S.  Blair,  Superintendent  of  the  Experimental  Station,  Kentville, 
took  a  keen  personal  interest  in  the  work  and  without  his  continuous  assistance 
an  important  phase  of  our  studies  could  never  have  been  carried  out.  His  efforts 
were  ably  seconded  in  many  ways  by  those  of  his  assistant,  Mr.  C.  C.  Eidt. 
Special  thanks  are  also  due  to  Mr.  F.  H.  Johnson  for  invaluable  assistance  during 
four  years  of  the  investigation;  to  Mr.  R.  D.  L.  Bligh  for  direction  of  pollination 
crew  during  1928;  to  Mr.  Evan  Craig  and  Mr.  H.  G.  Payne  for  essential  advice 
and  assistance  in  connection  with  apicultural  problems;  to  Messrs.  Robert  Long- 
ley,  John  Leefe,  and  Robert  Ward,  who  assisted  at  various  times  in  connection 
with  certain  phases  of  the  problem.  Mr.  A.  Hill  gave  invaluable  assistance  and 
advice  in  the  initial  vear  of  the  investigations.  Various  members  of  the  staff 
of  Acadia  University, 'including  Dr.  H.  G.  Perry,  Dr.  H.  W.  Harkness,  Dr.  D.  V. 
hnl,  assisted  with  equipment  or  advice,  while  Dr.  Muriel  V.  Roscoe  carried  out 
essential  cytological  studies  in  connection  with  the  chromosome  count  oi  apple 
varieties.  Permission  to  use  unpublished  data  secured  in  these  studies  was 
kindly  furnished  by  Dr.  Roscoe.  Mr.  J.  F.  Hockey  of  the  Dominion  Laboratory 
of  Plant  Pathology  extended  co-operation  in  the  form  of  the  use  of  equipment, 
laboratory  space  and  personal  assistance.  Mr.  M.  B.  Davis,  the  Dominion  De- 
partment of  Agriculture,  and  Dr.  C.  L.  Huskins,  oi  McGill  University,  read  and 


13 

criticized  parts  of  the  report.  In  the  studies  to  determine  sources  of  bee  poison- 
ing the  assistance  of  Mr.  H.  Groh  was  invaluable.  Mr.  J.  Patterson  assisted  us 
with  the  loan  of  the  solar  radiation  apparatus  used  in  our  experiments  and  Dr. 
W.  J.  Rowles  of  Macdonald  College  designed  and  prepared  the  photoelectric  bee 
counter.  Mr.  W.  E.  Whitehead  is  responsible  for  most  of  the  illustrations  used 
in  the  report.  To  all  members  of  the  Committee,  especially  those  who  actively 
assisted  in  the  investigation,  the  officer  in  charge  extends  his  sincere  thanks. 

I.  INTRODUCTION 

W.  H.  BRITTAIN 


A.  THE  APPLE  INDUSTRY   IN  NOVA   SCOTIA 

1.  GEOGRAPHICAL  POSITION 

The  province  of  Nova  Scotia  consists  of  a  peninsula  projecting  into  the 
Atlantic  ocean,  together  with  the  island  of  Cape  Breton.  It  lies  in  a  north- 
easterly and  southwesterly  direction  and  is  nearly  400  miles  in  length.  The 
forty-fifth  parallel  of  north  latitude  divides  the  province  into  two  nearly  equal 
parts.    The  "  fruit  belt  "  lies  in  the  western  part,  mostly  south  of  this  parallel, 


Fig.  1. — Map  of  Nova  Scotia  showing  fruit  belt   (original). 

in  what  is  generally  called  the  "  Annapolis  valley,"  though  this  district  really 
comprises  the  valleys  of  several  streams  emptying  into  the  bay  of  Fundy,  includ- 
ing the  Annapolis,  Cornwallis,  Gaspereaux  and  Avon  rivers.  A  range  of  hills 
500  to  700  feet  in  height  called  the  North  Mountain,  protects  the  Valley  from 
the  north  and  northeast  winds  and  forms  the  northern  boundary  of  the  Valley, 


14 


15 

while  a  corresponding  range  of  hills,  called  the  South  Mountain,  shuts  it  in  on 
the  south.  The  extreme  eastern  and  western  limits  are  marked  by  the  towns 
of  Annapolis  Royal  on  the  west  and  Windsor  on  the  east.  Of  course  there  are 
apples  grown  outside  of  this  area,  but,  in  the  main,  fruit  growing  on  a  large 
commercial  scale  is  largely  confined  to  this  territory.  Here  we  have  concen- 
trated in  a  valley  about  100  miles  long  and  varying  in  width  from  five  to  ten 
miles,  an  acreage  in  apple  trees  of  approximately  38,000  acres. 

2.   CLIMATE 

Fraser  (1924),  in  discussing  the  situation  of  important  apple  growing  regions 
of  North  America,  states  as  follows: — 

"  The  variation  between  the  day  and  night  temperatures  in  New  Jersey, 
Delaware  and  the  eastern  part  of  Maryland  and  Virginia  is  about  the  same 
as  that  along  the  shores  of  lake  Ontario.  In  both  instances  we  find  fruit 
regions  of  the  highest  rank.  The  Annapolis  valley  in  Nova  Scotia,  alongside 
the  bay  of  Fundy,  the  eastern  shore  of  lake  Michigan  and  the  Pacific  coast 
states  are  all  fruit  regions  because  of  their  climate." 

Writing  regarding  general  climatic  conditions  of  Nova  Scotia,  W.  A. 
Middleton,  Provincial  Horticulturist,  states: — 

"  Extreme  heat  or  cold  is  not  experienced  in  the  commercial  apple  area  of 
the  province.  The  summer  temperature  occasionally  rises  to  about  90  degrees, 
while  that  of  the  winter  seldom  falls  below  zero.  The  average  summer  tem- 
perature is  62  degrees,  that  of  the  winter  about  25  degrees,  and  the  mean  annual 
temperature  about  43  degrees.  The  annual  rainfall  ranges  from  40  to  45  inches 
and  is  generally  well  spread  over  each  month  of  the  year." 

Further  details  regarding  climate  are  given  elsewhere. 

The  soil  varies  from  a  light  sandy  loam  to  clay  loam.  Along  the  North 
Mountain  range  and  at  the  east  and  west  ends  of  the  Valley  the  heavier  soils 
predominate,  while  elsewhere  lighter  soils  are  more  in  evidence,  some  of  the 
lightest  soils  being  in  the  centre  of  the  Valley. 


3.  VARIETIES  GROWN 

Comeau   (23)   gives  the  following  partial  list  of  apple  varieties  grown  in 
the  Valley :- 


"Alexander  (Emperor) 
Ailing  ton  Pippin 
Arabka 

Arkansas  Beauty- 
Baldwin 
Baxter 
Ben  Davis 
Bethel 
Bietigheimer 
Bismark 

Blenheim   Orange   Pippin 
Bottle  Greening 
Borkins 
Bough  Sweet 
Bramley  Seedling 
Calkin  Pippin 
Canada  Baldwin 
Canada   Red 
Chenango   Strawberry 
Clayton 
Clyde  Beauty 
Charles  Ross 
Colvert 
Cooper's  Market 


Cornish   Aromatic 

Cortland 

Cox  Orange 

Cranberry   Pippin 

Crimson  Beauty 

Duchess 

Dudle}' 

Danvers  Sweet 

Delicious 

Early  Harvest 

Esopus  Spitzenburg 

Fall  Pippin 

Fallawater 

Fameuse    (Snow) 

Gano 

Gilliflower 

Gideon 

Gloria   Mundi 

Golden  Pippin 

Golden    Russet    (American) 

Golden  Russet    (English) 

Golden   Sweet 

Gravenstein 

Grimes  Golden 


Haas  (Fall  Queen) 

Honey  Sweet  (Winter  Sweet 

Paradise) 
Hubbardston 
Hublon 
Hunt  Russet 
Hurlbut 
Ingram 
Jacob  Sweet 
Jenneting 
Jersey  Sweet 
Jewett  Red 
Jonathan 
Jones 

Kent  Pippin 
Keswick  Codlin 
Kitchener 
King 

Lady  Finger 
Lady  Sweet 
Late  Strawberry 
Longfield 
Longworth 
Louise    (Princess  Louise) 


16 


3.  VARIETIES  GROWN— Concluded 


Mcintosh 

McMahon 

Maiden  Blush 

Mann 

Mother 

Xewtown   Pippin    (X.Y. 

Pippin) 
Nonpareil  (Roxbury  Russet) 
Northern  Spy 
North  West  Greening 
Ohio  Pippin 
Ontario 

Orange  (of  New  Jersey) 
Patten  Greening 
Pewaukee 
Peck  Pleasant 
Pennock 
Pine  Apple 
Porter  Pippin 
Pound  Sweet 
Pumpkin    Sweet 
Red  Streak 


Red    Sweet    Pippin    (Moore 

Sweet) 
Red  Russet 
R.I.  Greening 
Ribston  Pippin 
Rolfe 

Rome  Beauty 
Rose  Red 
Salome 

Scarlet  Pippin 
Scott  Winter 

Seek-no-further     (Westfield) 
Shackleford 
Shiawassee 
Smokehouse 
Stark 

Sutton   Beauty 
Swaar 
Swazie 

Sweet  Greening 
Thompson 


Tolman  Sweet 

Twenty  Ounce  (Cayuga  Red 

Streak) 
Twenty    Ounce    Pippin 
Vandevere     (Newton     Spitz- 

enburg) 
Victoria 
Wagener 
Wealthy 
Wellington 
Western  Beauty 
White  Apple 
White  Craft 
Winesap 

Williams'  Favorite 
Wilson's  Red  June 
Winter  Banana 
Winter  Bough 
Winter  Pippin 
Wolf  River 
Yellow   Bellflower 

(Bishop  Pippin) 
York  Imperial" 

"  Of  these  varieties,  the  following,  arranged  approximately  in  the  order  of 
ripening,  are  of  commercial  importance  in  the  fruit  district  of  Nova  Scotia: 

Wolf  River  R.  I.  Greening 

Mcintosh  Baldwin 

Blenheim  Stark 

Ribston  Northern    Spy 

King  Fallawater 

Bishop    Pippin  Golden   Russet 

Wagener  Nonpareil 

Cox  Orange  Ben  Davis  " 
Wellington 


Crimson  Beauty 
Yellow    Transparent 
Red   Astrachan 
Duchess  of  Oldenburg 
Williams'  Favorite 
Wealthy 
Gravenstein 
Dudley 
Alexander 


"  The  following  varieties  are  important  in  the  export  trade  of  the  province 

Gravenstein  Cox  Orange  Northern  Spy 

Blenheim  Wellington  Fallawater 

Ribston  R.  I.  Greening  Golden    Russet 

King  Baldwin  Nonpareil 

Wagener  Stark  Ben  Davis " 

"The  ten  best  commercial  varieties  for  export  are  as  follows: 


Golden   Russel 
Nonpareil  "' 


for    commercial    plantings    are    as 
Spy  (Red) 

Baldwin 

but  mainly  for  local  demand-. 


Gravenstein  Wagener 

Blenheim  R.   I.  Greening 

Ribston  Baldwin 

King  Northern  Spy 

The  officially   recommended    varieties 
follows : 

Gravenstein    (Crimson)  Golden  Russet 

Cox  Orange  King   (Red) 

Mcintosh  is  favoured  by  some  authorities, 

The  standard  varieties  that  are  recommended  to  be  retained,  but  not  recom- 
mended for  future  planting  are  as  follows: 
Ribston  Pippin  Nonpareil  Wolf  River 

Blenheim   Orange  Wellington  Gano 

Stark  Wealthy  Ben  Davis 

Greening  Bramley  Seedling 

Of  the  foregoing  Wagener  is  favoured  as  a  filler  owing  to  the  small  >'\ze  of 
the  tree  and  the  fact  that  it  is  a  short  lived  tree.  It  has  an  additional  advantage 
in  being  an  effective  pollinizer  for  the  early  varieties 


17 

4.  QUANTITY  GROWN 

The  following  tabulation  taken  from  official  sources  indicates  the  growth 
of  the  apple  industry  for  the  past  half  century: 

Barrels 

1880-85 — Annual   Average    30,320 

1885-90—  "  "           .  . . 83,356 

1890-95—  "  "           118,556 

1895-1900—  "  " 261,879 

1900-05—  "  "           377,225 

1905-10—  "  "           496.655 

1910-15—  "  "           786,633 

1915-20—  "  "           932,957 

1920-28—  "  "           1,056,057 

1929—  "  "           2,134,100 

1930—  "  "           1,172,443 

1931—  "  "           1,611,273 

In  this  connection  a  tabulation  of  the  actual  produced  crop  of  the  different 
varieties  grown  in  the  Valley  is  of  interest: — 

PERCENTAGE  CROP  OF  VARIETIES  OF  APPLES  OF  THE  TOTAL  CROP  PRODUCED 
IN  NOVA  SCOTIA    (FIGURES  BY  UNITED  FRUIT  CO.) 

Gravenstein 13 

Baldwin 13 

Stark 10 

Ben  Davis 10 

Odd  varieties 9 

Ribston 8 

King 6.5 

Northern  Spy 6 

Nonpareil  (Roxbury  Russet) 5 

Blenheim 5 

Golden  Russet 5 

Fallawater •..    ..  2.5 

Wagener 2 

Gano 1.6 

Greening 1.5 

Wolf  River 0.8 

Wealthy 0.7 

Bishop  Pippin  (Yellow  Bellflower) 0.4 

100.0 

On  examination  of  these  figures,  it  is  apparent  that  approximately  25  per 
cent  of  the  crop  is  Gravenstein,  Ribston  and  King;  25  per  cent  Blenheim, 
Northern  Spy,  Nonpareil,  Golden  Russet,  Fallawater  and  Wagener;  35  per  cent 
Baldwin,  Ben  Davis,  Gano  and  Stark,  and  15  per  cent  other  sorts.  This  shows 
that  85  per  cent  of  the  crop  is  made  up  of  what  we  consider  13  standard  sorts. 

From  the  foregoing  data  it  is  seen  that  a  very  large  number  of  varieties 
are  grown  in  Nova  Scotia.  One  of  the  chief  problems  of  the  fruit  industry  is 
to  reduce  the  number  of  unprofitable  varieties,  but  it  is  only  within  recent  years 
that  the  movement  to  cut  down  the  number  of  varieties  planted  and  to  graft 
out  the  non-commercial  and  unprofitable  sorts  has  gained  headway.  In  connec- 
tion with  this  movement  it  is  of  interest  to  note  that  85  per  cent  of  the  crop  is 
made  up  of  what  may  be  considered  standard  varieties. 

5.  AVERAGE   YIELDS 

It  has  been  estimated  that  in  the  period  1900-1905  the  average  yield  of 
apples  in  Nova  Scotia  was  16  barrels  per  acre;  in  the  period  1910-1915  it  was 
24^  barrels  per  acre,  while  the  average  annual  yield  in  the  years  1920-1926 
was  30  barrels  per  acre.  Included  in  these  figures  there  is,  of  course,  a  large 
acreage  of  small,  non-commercial  orchards,  though,  even  with  all  allowances,  the 
average  crop  must  be  regarded  as  small.  The  figures  for  a  number  of  typical 
orchards  from  central  Kings  county,  which  is  the  most  typical  orchard  section, 

60796—2 


18 

including  both  poorly  cared  for  and  well  cared  for  orchards,  indicate  that  in 
really  commercial  orchards  the  crop  is  much  more  satisfactory. 

An  examination  of  the  figures  shows  that,  in  the  period  under  study,  the 
yield  per  acre  varied  from  26  to  172  barrels,  while  the  average  for  all  these 
orchards  was  considerably  higher  than  the  average  yield  for  the  province,  viz., 
66-6  barrels.  Average  yields  of  from  80  to  100  barrels  per  acre  are  considered 
necessary  for  profitable  fruit  growing  and  several  orchardists  have  attained 
this  figure,  but  the  number  that  fall  below  this  amount  materially  lowers  the 
average  figures. 


B.   FACTORS   OTHER   THAN   POLLINATION   AFFECTING 
FRUIT  PRODUCTION 

1.  GENERAL 

Since  this  report  is  concerned  with  the  problem  of  pollination,  it  is  not 
necessary  to  deal  exhaustively  with  other  factors  involved  in  the  setting  or 
production  of  apples.  Nevertheless,  a  brief  discussion  of  certain  of  the  more 
important  factors  involved  in  this  process,  may  enable  us  better  to  appreciate 
the  relative  importance  of  the  pollination  factor  and  the  phenomena  associated 
therewith,  and  the  better  to  evaluate  the  results  of  the  pollination  studies 
described  herein. 

It  is  well  known  to  all  students  of  the  subject  that  the  production  of 
profitable  crops  of  marketable  fruit  is  the  end  result  of  a  long  series  of  factors. 
Pollination  is  but  a  link,  though  a  very  important  link,  in  the  chain.  Some  of 
these  factors  are  at  least  partially  under  the  control  of  the  grower;  many  are 
not.  The  proper  procedure  for  every  fruit-grower  should  be  to  keep  all  condi- 
tions as  favourable  as  possible  for  the  production  of  large  crops  of  quality  fruit, 
so  that,  through  neglect  to  attend  to  a  single  factor,  he  may  not  lose  not  only  his 
crop,  but  also  the  money  spent  in  attempting  to  produce  it.  The  proper  atten- 
tion to  all  factors  within  his  power  to  control  may  enable  him  to  reduce  or 
minimize  the  losses  resulting  from  unfavourable  conditions  not  directly  con- 
trollable. 

In  order  to  secure  dependable  data  with  regard  to  all  measurable  and 
relevant  factors  we  have  tabulated  the  weather  records  for  the  growing  season 
over  a  period  of  years  and  have  attempted  to  analyze  these  data  from  the  stand- 
point of  the  possible  effect  upon  fruit  setting  and  fruit  production.  An  intensive 
study  of  16  representative  orchards  situated  in  central  Kings  county  has  also 
been  made  over  a  three-year  period  in  an  attempt  to  determine,  if  possible,  the 
effect  of  various  cultural  practices.  In  addition,  the  crop  returns  of  34  growers 
for  a  period  of  six  years  were  compiled  and  studied  in  connection  with  their 
expenditure  for  spray  material,  fertilizer,  etc..  as  obtained  from  the  records  of 
a  local  fruit  company.  Furthermore,  an  economic  survey  of  approximately  one 
hundred  orchards  in  the  same  district  was  made.  In  this  survey  special  atten- 
tion was  again  given  to  the  question  of  fertilizer  and  spraying  practices,  as  these 
factors  lent  themselves  well  to  statistical  treatment  and  gave  indication  of  being 
of  considerable  importance  in  connection  with  the  quantity  of  apples  produced. 
These  surveys  were  carried  out  by  Mr.  Robert  Longley  of  the  Provincial  staff 
and  only  the  more  outstanding  and  general  conclusions  are  referred  to  in  this 
report.  The  results  obtained  are  in  general  agreement  with  the  more  prolonged 
studies  conducted  by  the  Economics  Branch  of  the  Dominion  Department  of 
Agriculture  and  reported  by  Coke  (1931). 


19 
2.   CLIMATIC   FACTORS 

The  most  important  factor,  or  rather  complex  of  factors  not  under  the 
control  of  man  is  that  conveniently  summarized  under  the  heading  of  "  climate  ". 
The  effect  of  certain  climatic  factors  is  easy  to  observe,  but,  in  the  main,  these 
factors  are  difficult  to  measure  and  hard  to  evaluate. 

The  effect  of  climate  can  best  be  discussed  with  reference  to  (1)  its  direct 
effect  upon  the  blossoms,  or  (2)  its  indirect  effect  upon  the  insect  pollinators. 
Of  these  the  second  is  probably  more  important,  but  since  this  problem  is  dis- 
cussed at  great  length  elsewhere,  it  is  merely  referred  to  at  this  point. 

(a)  WEATHER    CONDITIONS    DURING    BLOOM 

Careful  records  of  weather  during  the  blossoming  period  have  been  kept 
for  a  number  of  years,  but  it  is  difficult  to  establish  significant  correlations 
between  such  conditions  and  the  set  of  fruit,  probably  because  conditions  at 
a  certain  critical  period  such  as  the  period  of  stigma  receptivity  may  be  the 
crucial  factor  and  our  methods  have  not  achieved  a  sufficient  degree  of  refine- 
ment to  demonstrate  this.  However,  certain  facts  are  sufficiently  clear  to 
deserve  mention. 

Frost  during  the  bloom  may  result  in  the  destruction  of  the  stigmatic 
surfaces  of  the  pistil  followed  by  the  subsequent  drop  of  the  blossoms.  Such 
frosts  are  of  comparatively  rare  occurrence  in  most  parts  of  the  Valley.  Since 
the  inception  of  this  investigation,  no  such  losses  have  occurred  in  a  series  of 
orchards  in  which  observations  have  been  made,  except  in  one  case  in  1932 
in  an  orchard  known  to  be  in  a  "  frost  belt  ".  Such  conditions  do  occasion- 
ally occur,  particularly  in  certain  sections  in  the  centre  of  the  Valley.  It  has 
been  pointed  out  (Murneek,  1930)  that  frosty  weather  at  bloom,  even  where 
it  does  not  kill,  may  prevent  the  growth  of  the  pollen  tube. 

Hedrick  (1908)  contends  that  a  temperature  slightly  above  the  average  is 
usually  most  favourable  for  fruit  setting.  On  the  other  hand,  a  very  high  tem- 
perature, especially  if  accompanied  by  wind,  may  bring  about  the  drying  up  of 
the  stigmas,  so  that  they  remain  receptive  for  but  a  short  time.  It  was  note- 
worthy that  in  the  season  of  1930  the  blossoming  period  was  coincident  with 
a  very  high  temperature  and  a  short  period  of  stigma  receptivity  was  noted  by 
all  workers,  whereas,  during  the  cooler,  more  humid  season  of  1931,  the  period  of 
stigma  receptivity  was  longer.  Knowlton  (1929)  states  that,  "  it  is  an  estab- 
lished horticultural  fact  that  a  larger  set  of  fruit  occurs  on  selfed  varieties  in 
seasons  when  the  temperatures  are  most  favourable  for  pollen  tube  growth." 
When  we  study  the  wide  range  of  self-fruitfulness  that  may  occur  in  the  same 
variety  from  year  to  year,  we  are  forced  to  believe  that  this  must  be  the  case. 
This  was  particularly  noticeable  in  Blenheim  in  the  favourable  blooming  period 
of  1931.  We  had  become  accustomed  to  regard  this  variety  as  almost  com- 
pletely self-unfruitful,  but  in  1932  selfing  produced  a  fruit  set  of  up  to  five  per 
cent. 

Excessive  humidity  and  driving  rains  during  bloom  appear  to  have  an 
adverse  effect  upon  the  pollen,  which  may  be  washed  away  or  deteriorate  while 
still  on  the  anthers,  or  they  may  even  wash  off  pollen  already  deposited  on  the 
stigma.  Low  yields  from  certain  varieties  have  been  observed  to  follow  periods 
of  rainy  weather  during  the  blossoming  periods  of  such  varieties.  The  occur- 
rence of  high  winds  during  bloom,  often  mentioned  as  a  factor  detrimental  to 
fruit  setting,  has  been  a  negligible  factor  during  the  course  of  these  studies  in 
four  out  of  the  five  years,  but  in  1932  it  appeared  to  be  of  importance  in  certain 
sections. 

The  fact  that  it  may  not  be  necessary  for  more  than  one  blossom  in  twenty 
to  develop  into  a  marketable  apple  in  order  to  secure  a  commercial  crop,  gives 

60796— 2  J 


20 


a  considerable  margin  of  safety  in  apple  growing,  since,  even  though  a  large 
proportion  of  blossoms  may  be  destroyed,  the  set  may  be  sufficiently  large,  and, 
in  many  cases,  larger  than  necessary  for  a  commercial  crop.  This  fact  is  brought 
out  by  the  records  of  fruit  setting  for  four  standard  varieties  in  sixteen  orchards 
for  the  years  1928-1930  inclusive,  taken  together  with  the  weather  records  for 
the  same  period. 

TABLE  No.  1.— RECORD  OF  PER  CENT  FRUIT  OBTAINED  IN  SIXTEEN  KINGS  COUNTY 

ORCHARDS,  1928-1930 


Variety 

1928 

1929 

1930 

Average 

Gravenstein 

3-98 
510 
6-90 
7-08 

5-64 

4-49 

11-32 

9-13 

7  05 

7-79 

10-20 

10-65 

5-86 

King 

5-68 

9-92 

Spy 

10-12 

5-76 

7-64 

8-92 

7-64 

Note. —  "Per  cent  fruit"  refers  to  the  percentage  of  blossoms  that  produced  fruit  which  remained 
after  the  "July  drop".  Figures  based  on  counts  of  approximately  1,000  blossoms  of  each  variety  each 
year. 

The  figures  would  seem  to  indicate  that  in  these  orchards  during  the  period 
studied  the  average  set  obtained  was  at  least  reasonably  satisfactory.  Unfor- 
tunately, this  period  does  not  include  a  season  similar  to  1932.  when  conditions 
for  pollination  were  very  unfavourable  owing  to  the  very  broken  weather.  It 
would  be  expected  that  under  such  conditions  failure  to  set  through  lack  of  a 
proper  distribution  of  pollinizing  varieties  and  an  inadequate  force  of  insect 
pollinators  would  be  accentuated. 

(6)  WEATHER  CONDITIONS  AT  OTHER  PERIODvS 

Winter  injury  to  trees  of  the  ordinary  commercial  varieties  is  not  common 
in  the  fruit  belt  of  Nova  Scotia,  but  freezing  weather  that  occurs  after  the  buds 
have  started  in  the  spring  but  before  the  blossoms  open,  may  act  directly  on 
the  blossoms,  destroying  the  essential  parts  of  the  flower.  Indirect  injury  result- 
ing in  a  reduced  crop  may  be  caused  by  frost  injury  to  the  developing  leaves. 
Such  injury  results  in  a  dwarfing  and  curling  of  the  leaf,  which  is  inclined  to  be 
brittle  and,  not  infrequently,  the  lower  epidermis  separates  from  the  overlying 
cells.  Such  a  condition  was  quite  widespread  in  1928  and  occurred  locally  in 
1932.  It  should  be  noted  that  the  destruction  of  even  a  considerable  proportion 
of  blossom  buds  by  frost,  does  not  necessarily  reduce  the  final  crop,  since  suffi- 
cient may  set  from  the  uninjured  buds  to  produce  a  commercial  yield.  The  injury 
to  the  primary  leaves  is  sometimes  more  serious  and  sometimes  appears  to  result 
in  an  abnormal  drop. 

The  climate  of  the  Annapolis  valley  is  ordinarily  characterized  by  an  ade- 
quate supply  of  moisture  throughout  the  growing  season,  and  protracted  droughts 
resulting  in  dwarfing  and  even  drop  of  leaves  and  fruit  are  comparatively  rare. 
They  are,  however,  not  entirely  unknown,  as,  for  example,  in  the  seasons  of  1921 
and  1928,  where  orchards  on  dry  land  suffered  badly,  in  some  cases,  and  again 
in  1930,  when  a  dry  period  of  about  eight  weeks  during  the  autumn  months 
resulted  in  considerable  dwarfing  of  the  fruit. 

Prolonged  periods  of  rainy  weather  have  rarely  been  mentioned  as  a  cause 
of  crop  reduction.  It  would  appear,  however,  on  the  basis  of  data  secured  during 
these  investigations  that  the  effect  of  excessive  rainfall  during  the  growing  season 
had  a  detrimental  effect  on  crop  production,  whereas  the  greater  the  sunlight 
during  the  months  May  to  July  inclusive,  the  more  favourable  the  condition  for 
fruit  production.  An  extended  discussion  of  this  problem,  however,  is  beyond 
the  scope  of  this  paper. 


21 

All  the  preceding  discussion  has  had  to  do  with  the  effect  of  weather  con- 
ditions on  the  crop  of  the  corresponding  year.  That  the  climatic  complex  also 
has  an  effect  upon  the  crop  of  subsequent  seasons  is  equally  true,  though  the 
relative  importance  of  the  different  factors  is  hard  to  evaluate.  The  fact  that, 
in  certain  seasons,  apple  trees  everywhere,  even  wild  or  uncared-for  trees,  bloom 
heavily,  as  in  1929  and  in  1931,  is  indicative  of  the  effect  of  little  understood 
climatic  factors  in  fruit-bud  formation  and  vigour. 

Significant  weather  records  are  presented  in  table  2,  together  with  crop 
ngures  for  the  corresponding  years. 

TABLE  No.  2.— WEATHER  CONDITIONS  FOR  GROWING  SEASON 


Per  cent 

May-July  (inclusive)  and  April-October  (inclusive) 

possible 

Year 

Barrels 

bloom 

Mean 

Total 

Total 

Mean 

Total 

Total 

(esti- 

tem- 

ram 

sunlight 

tem- 

rain 

sunlight 

mated) 

perature 

(ins.) 

(hrs.) 

perature 

(ins.) 

(hrs.) 

1915 

613,882 

9 

55-73 

12-05 

593-5 

54-5 

23-09 

1,271-9 

1916 

631,470 

60 

55-79 

6-67 

611-9 

54-6 

28-04 

1,403-5 

1917 

774,730 

80 

56-65 

8-90 

513-2 

53-0 

27-35 

1,230-4 

1918 

827,693 

45 

59-11 

7-85 

667-0 

54-8 

21-94 

1,434-6 

1919 

1 , 600, 000 

90 

58-17 

6-44 

688-8 

54-4 

16-09 

1,357-8 

1920 

1,167,000 

65 

56-20 

6-00 

792-1 

54-73 

17-03 

1,528-1 

1921 

2,000,000 

85 

58-17 

5-34 

700-4 

55-92 

13-98 

1,511-3 

1922 

1,800,000 

80 

57-82 

9-14 

599-4 

56-14 

21-37 

1,255-5 

1923 

2,021,177 

90 

55-64 

7-26 

625-6 

53-30 

20-92 

1,350-4 

1924 

1,976,340 

85 

59-07 

5-55 

762-6 

54-80 

18-55 

1,492-7 

1925 

1,447,401 

75 

58-88 

10-77 

652-7 

53-53 

22-88 

1,378-7 

1926 

1,033,021 

65 

55-17 

9-73 

668-9 

52-18 

19-93 

1,420-4 

1927 

1,086,932 

85 

56-67 

10-37 

665-3 

54-24 

30-11 

1,405-7 

1928 

1,244,987 

45 

58-70 

7-44 

631-2 

55-50 

18-33 

1,310-3 

1929 

2,134,100 

95 

59-34 

9-26 

775-5 

55-57 

19-89 

1,459-0 

1930 

1,172,443 

65 

61-66 

6-33 

658-1 

56  05 

13-58 

1,509-6 

1931 

1,611,273 

95 

59-75 

7-73 

620-3 

56-23 

21-64 

1,321-5 

1932 

60 

1 

3.  NUTRITIONAL  FACTORS 

The  question  of  the  nutritional  factors  that  affect  fruiting  is  too  technical 
for  extended  treatment  in  a  paper  dealing  with  pollination  and  is  neither 
necessary  nor  desirable.  Nevertheless,  it  is  evident  that  the  set  of  fruit  may 
be  influenced  jointly  by  pollination  and  a  number  of  these  other  factors. 
Trees  lacking  in  vigour  are  incapable  of  setting  large  crops  whether  pollinated 
or  not,  and  it  is  not  surprising  that  many  workers  have  stressed  tree  vigour, 
and,  more  especially,  fruit-spur  vigour,  as  a  factor  in  fruit  setting.  This  factor 
is  important  because  vigorous  spurs  can  better  supply  an  abundance  of  water 
and  organic  nutrients  to  the  developing  fruit.  MacDaniels  and  Heinicke  (1929) 
present  evidence  to  show  that  there  may  be  greater  need  for  cross-pollina- 
tion, which  is  usually  a  requisite  for  seed  formation,  when  the  trees  have  produced 
a  large  proportion  of  weak  spurs,  or  when  they  are  growing  under  conditions 
which  otherwise  limit  the  sap  and  nutrient  supply  to  flowers  and  young  fruits. 
The  maintenance  of  a  high  level  of  nutrition  may  partially  compensate  for 
imperfect  pollination  or  for  irregularities  in  chromosome  behaviour  at  megasporo- 
genesis  as  Howlett  (1932)  contends. 

Lattimer  (1931)  noted  in  pollination  experiments  conducted  by  him,  that, 
in  less  vigorous  trees  there  is  a  greater  difference  in  relative  effectiveness 
between  the  better  pollinizers.  On  more  vigorous  trees  there  was  little  difference 
between  the  several  good  pollinizers  used.  The  conclusion  is  drawn  that  when 
low-vigour  trees  are  to  be  pollinated  one  should  choose  only  an  effective  pollin- 
izer.  The  different  results  obtained  on  various  trees  and  between  different  limbs 
of  the  same  tree  have  been  brought  out  repeatedly  in  our  tests. 


22 

Sandsten  (1909)  has  recorded  that  the  condition  of  the  tree  may  have  an 
important  bearing  on  the  quality  of  the  pollen,  it  being  inferior  from  orchards  in 
poor  cultural  condition.  Tree  and  fruit-spur  vigour  is  the  result  of  proper 
nutrition  and  this  has  to  do  with  cultural  practices  and  the  maintenance  of  a 
suitable  supply  of  moisture  and  nutrients.  The  importance  of  an  adequate 
supply  of  nitrogen  and  the  question  of  maintaining  a  correct  proportion  between 
the  nitrogen  and  carbohydrates  in  the  plant,  is  a  matter  to  which  much  attention 
has  been  given  during  the  past  few  years.  The  varying  results  obtained  from 
tests  with  the  same  pollen  on  different  limbs  and  even  on  different  spurs 
emphasize  the  importance  of  the  factor  of  vigour. 

Anything  that  upsets  this  balance  in  the  tree  has  a  tendency  to  reduce  fruit 
setting,  though  judicious  pruning  may  increase  it.  Excessive  applications  of 
nitrogenous  fertilizer,  if  ever  given,  appear  to  be  rare.  Our  surveys  indicate  that 
applications  of  from  500  to  600  pounds  per  acre  not  only  tend  to  increase  setting, 
but  are  commercially  profitable.  The  coefficient  of  correlation  existing  between 
the  applications  of  commercial  nitrogenous  fertilizer,  mainly  in  the  form  of 
nitrate  of  soda,  and  crop  produced  was  -4844  ±  -0527  for  the  orchards  used 
in  our  economic  survey.  It  is  believed  that  this  correlation  would  have  been 
much  higher,  if  allowance  could  have  been  made  for  the  effect  of  previous  crops. 
All  data  secured  tend  to  emphasize  the  value  of  an  adequate  supply  of  readily 
available  nitrogen.  Our  figures  indicated  no  value  as  far  as  crop  was  concerned 
for  nitrogen  applied  in  the  form  of  barnyard  manure,  though  the  presence  of  an 
adequate  supply  of  organic  matter  in  the  soil  is  undoubtedly  beneficial  and 
should  by  no  means  be  neglected  in  actual  orchard  practice.  Otherwise,  the 
beneficial  effect  of  commercial  nitrogenous  fertilizer  may  not  be  experienced. 
Regarding  the  value  of  other  types  of  fertilizers  we  do  not  have  sufficient  infor- 
mation from  our  own  studies,  though  the  advantage  of  using  a  balanced  fertilizer 
has  been  insisted  upon  by  certain  investigators.  That  low  phosphorus  may  be 
an  important  limiting  factor  has  been  demonstrated  in  other  tests. 

Our  survey  indicates  that  good  crops  are  possible  both  under  conditions 
of  sod  mulch  or  clean  culture,  if  properly  carried  out,  and  any  system  of  treat- 
ment that  results  in  a  strong  growth  of  spurs  and  provides  for  an  accumulation 
of  stored  food  materials  in  the  spur,  is  considered  to  favour  fruit  setting.  Best 
crops  are  secured  where  the  trees  produce  a  good  growth  of  leaves,  terminal 
growth  and  trunk. 

4.  PATHOLOGICAL  FACTORS 

Under  this  heading  we  consider  the  injuries  caused  by  insects  and  fungous 
pests.  These  may  influence  the  crop  indirectly  by  injuring  the  leaves,  resulting 
in  dwarfing  or  even  drop  of  fruit,  or  they  directly  injure  the  blossoms  or  the 
fruit  itself.  The  apple  scab  organism  (Venturia  inaequalis  (Cook)  Winter),  is 
one  of  the  most  important  orchard  pests.  Not  only  do  heavy  infestations  result 
in  leaf-injury,  causing,  in  seven1  cases,  dwarfing  and  partial  defoliation,  but 
early  infection  causes  drop  of  blossoms  or  young  fruit  by  its  development  upon 
the  flower  stalks. 

Of  the  insects  responsible  for  reducing  fruit  setting  the  green  apple  bug 
(Lygus  communis  Knight),  is  of  most  interest  from  our  standpoint,  because, 
owing  to  its  small  size,  it  is  often  overlooked  and  its  injury  attributed  to  other 
causes,  including  lack  of  pollination.  The  bud-moth  (Tmctoccra  occllana 
iSchiff.),  also  deserves  special  mention.  Many  other  pests  might  be  mentioned 
in  this  connection,  but  those  already  indicated  are  sufficient  for  purposes  of 
illustration. 

A  study  of  the  yield  records  from  sprayed  and  unsprayed  orchards  over  a 
long  period  of  years,  shows  a  decided  advantage  in  yield  as  well  as  in  quality 
of  fruit,  in  favour  of  the  sprayed  orchards.     The  result  of  our  economic  survey 


23 

showed  a  significant  correlation  between  spray  costs  and  yield,  there  being  an 
upward  trend  of  crops  as  the  amount  of  spray  materials  used  was  increased. 
Confirmation  of  these  results  is  obtained  from  a  similar  survey  carried  out  by 
the  Agricultural  Economics  Branch  of  the  Department  of  Agriculture  (Coke, 
1931),  in  which  it  is  likewise  shown  that  crops  and  financial  returns  per  acre 
increase  with  the  amount  expended  upon  pest  control. 

Against  the  beneficial  effects  of  spraying  and  dusting  must  be  set  the  injury 
that  occasionally  results  from  the  use  of  certain  spray  schedules,  but  an  examina- 
tion of  our  figures  proves  that  even  those  materials  reputed  to  be  most  danger- 
ous, show,  over  a  period  of  years,  a  significant  increase  in  crop  over  untreated 
trees. 

The  indirect  effect  of  poison  applications  to  bloom  upon  insect  pollinators  is 
discussed  at  length  elsewhere,  but  the  effect  of  the  not  uncommon  practice  of 
applying  a  fungicide  dust  during  bloom,  especially  in  seasons  when  the  blossom- 
ing period  has  been  prolonged,  may  be  conveniently  referred  to  at  this  point.  The 
apparent  injurious  effect  upon  stigmas  and  anthers  of  applications  of  various 
fungicidal  and  insecticidal  preparations  has  long  been  noted.  In  1930  an  applica- 
tion of  sulphur  dust  to  Golden  Russet  in  our  experiments  reduced  the  original 
set  by  about  10  per  cent  but  did  not  lower  the  final  fruit  yield.  Since  apple 
trees  do  not  bloom  evenly,  only  a  certain  proportion  of  blossoms  are  in  a  con- 
dition to  be  injured  at  any  one  time.  Therefore,  the  destruction  of  a  certain  pro- 
portion of  blossoms  may  even  be  beneficial  in  imparting  greater  vigour  to  those 
that  remain,  as  shown  by  MacDaniels  and  Furr  (1931)  in  the  case  of  sulphur 
dust.  If,  on  the  other  hand,  the  application  is  made  early,  before  many  blos- 
soms are  pollinated,  or  before  the  pollen  tube  has  penetrated  the  style  sufficiently 
to  be  beyond  the  effect  of  the  sulphur,  an  injurious  effect  upon  set  may  result 
on  certain  varieties.  Also  with  varieties  that  tend  to  produce  only  one  or  two 
fruits  to  the  spur  such  as  King,  the  result  may  be  more  serious  than  on  varieties 
that  tend  to  set  in  clusters,  as  Wagener,  Baldwin,  etc. 


II.  POLLINATION  AND  FRUITFULNESS  IN  APPLES 

W.   H.   BRITTAIN 


A.    THE   PROCESS    OF    POLLINATION   AND    FERTILIZATION 

(Definitions) 

Since  there  has  been  some  lack  of  uniformity  in  the  nomenclature  used  in 
connection  with  pollination  work  the  following  definitions  of  terms,  as  employed 
throughout  this  paper,  are  given: 

Pollination. — The  mere  transfer  of  the  pollen  from  anther  to  stigma  con- 
stitutes pollination  and  is  usually  effected  by  the  aid  of  insects.  The  transfer  of 
pollen  from  the  anther  of  one  flower  of  one  variety  to  the  stigma  of  another  of 
the  same  variety  is  known  as  self-pollination.  The  transfer  from  the  anther  of 
one  flower  of  one  variety  to  the  stigma  of  another  is  cross-pollination. 

Pollinizer. — The  male  parent,  that  is,  the  plant  that  furnishes  the  pollen. 

Pollinator. — The  agent  for  distribution  of  the  pollen. 

As  all  investigators  have  shown,  the  chief  agents  in  the  transfer  of  pollen 
from  one  flower  to  the  other,  are  insects.  A  certain  amount  of  apple  pollen  is,  it 
is  true,  carried  by  wind,  as  will  be  discussed  at  greater  length  elsewhere,  but  wind 
pollination  as  far  as  the  apple  is  concerned,  is  negligible.  In  the  absence  of  the 
hive  bee  over  wide  areas  of  our  fruit  belt,  this  role  is  performed  largely  by  wild 
bees,  mostly  of  the  genera  Halictus  and  Andrena,  though  certain  species  of  flies 
and  certain  other  insects  play  a  minor  part. 

Fertilization. — Following  pollination  with  the  pollen  of  a  suitable  variety, 
the  pollen  tube  develops,  grows  down  the  style  through  the  tissue  and  finally 
reaches  the  ovary,  where,  upon  penetration  of  the  ovule,  the  sperm  is  discharged 
into  the  embryo  sac,  where  it  unites  with  the  egg  cell,  thus  accomplishing  fer- 
tilization, this  process  usually  resulting  in  the  formation  of  seed.  This  initiates 
growth  and  development  of  the  fruit  and  is  requisite  to  setting.  Where  the  ovules 
fail  of  fertilization,  or  where,  for  any  reason  development  is  checked  the  blossoms 
are  usually  shed. 

Fruitful. — A  plant  which  produces  mature  fruit  is  said  to  be  fruitful.  As 
ordinarily  used  it  means  sufficient  to  produce  a  commercial  crop. 

Self -fruitful  and  Self -unfruitful. — From  the  standpoint  of  their  requirement- 
for  pollination  apples  may  be  either  (1)  self -fruitful  or  (2)  self -unfruitful,  i.e. 
(1)  they  are  capable  of  producing  mature  fruit  when  pollinated  with  their  own 
pollen  or  (2)  they  require  the  pollen  from  another  variety  in  order  to  produce 
fruit.  We  shall  see  later  that  few,  if  any,  varieties  are  completely  self-unfruitful, 
and  hence  the  expression  "  partially  self-fruitful  "  largely  loses  its  meaning.  In 
fact,  self-unfruitfulness  is  rarely  completely,  expressed  in  the  apple,  as  pointed 
out  by  Crane  and  Lawrence,  varying  from  varieties  that  yield  little  or  no  fruit 
when  self-pollinated  to  others  that  produce  a  set  little  short  of  that  obtained 
from  favourable  cross-pollination. 

Cross-fruitful  and  Cross-unfruitful. — When  the  pollen  from  one  variety 
results  in  fruit  production  when  placed  on  the  stigma  of  another  variety,  the 
first  variety  is  said  to  be  cross-fruitful  with  the  second  variety.     On  the'  other 

24 


25 

hand,  if  fruit  does  not  result  the  condition  is  known  as  cross-unfruitful.  Just  as 
few  apple  varieties  are  completely  self-unfruitful,  so  there  will  be  few  cases  in 
which  thp  pollen  from  one  variety  is  completely  useless  for  another  variety, 
though  there  are  many  combinations  that  give  very  poor  results  commercially. 
Therefore,  the  expressions  "  commercially  self-  or  cross-fruitful "  would  be  more 
accurate. 

Sterility. — The  condition  in  which  there  is  failure  to  set  fruit  with  viable 
seed. 

Self-sterile  and  Self-fertile. — The  terms  self-sterile  and  self-fertile,  often 
used  synonymously  with  self-unfruitful  and  self-fruitful  respectively,  are  more 
properly  applied  to  the  ability  or  inability  of  the  plant  to  produce  fruit  with 
viable  seeds  when  self-pollinated.  Since  fruits  may  be  obtained  which  produce 
no  viable  seeds  in  the  case  of  certain  apple  varieties,  such  a  variety  may  be 
self-fruitful  and,  at  the  same  time  self-sterile,  at  least  theoretically.  The  pro- 
duction of  viable  seedlings  is,  of  course,  an  all-important  factor  in  breeding  work, 
but  is  not  of  direct  interest  in  connection  with  pollination  experiments,  except  in 
so  far  as  the  production  of  viable  seeds  is  to  be  correlated  with  the  value  of  cer- 
tain varieties  as  pollinizers  for  other  varieties. 

B.    THE  POLLINATION  PROBLEM 

It  will  be  clear  from  the  foregoing  that  the  pollination  problem  in  commer- 
cial orchards  consists  (1)  in  planting  together  varieties  that  are  inter-fruitful, 
i.e.,  in  which  each  variety  has  another  blooming  at  the  same  time  that  can  be 
depended  upon  to  pollinate  it,  or  (2)  in  "  working  over  "  a  sufficient  number 
of  trees  in  blocks  of  self-unfruitful  or  cross-unfruitful  varieties  to  secure  adequate 
pollination,  or  (3)  in  supplying  insect  pollinators  where  they  are  not  present  in 
adequate  numbers. 

It  has  been  shown  that,  in  Nova  Scotia,  a  great  many  varieties  are  grown 
and  most  of  the  older  orchards  are  planted  to  a  large  number  of  varieties.  While 
this  has  been  a  very  serious  drawback  from  a  commercial  standpoint,  it  has 
resulted  in  the  pollination  problem  being  less  serious  than  in  many  other  districts, 
where  the  exclusive  growth  of  a  few  commercially  desirable  sorts  has  been  the 
rule.  In  many  of  these  orchards,  for  practically  all  varieties  there  are  present 
others  with  which  they  are  cross-fruitful.  In  other  cases,  the  mixture  of 
varieties,  from  the  standpoint  of  pollination,  has  been  unsuitable  and  has 
undoubtedly  resulted  in  subnormal  crops,  especially  in  years  in  which  conditions 
for  cross-pollination  were  unfavourable.  Such  subnormal  crops  over  a  long 
period  of  years  may  well  result  in  a  large  aggregate  loss  to  the  grower.  In  a 
few  cases  large  blocks  of  self-unfruitful  varieties  have  been  set  out.  A  good 
example  is  Blenheim,  which  is  one  of  the  most  self-unfruitful  of  locally  grown 
apple  varieties.  Owing  to  its  resistance  to  apple  scab,  numbers  of  growers  set 
out  blocks  of  this  variety,  with  the  result  of  poor  and  infrequent  crops.  Con- 
siderable difficulty  has  also  been  experienced  with  Stark,  Ben  Davis  and,  to 
a  lesser  extent,  certain  other  varieties.  With  the  recent  movement  to  restrict 
new  planting  to  a  few  of  the  more  profitable  varieties  and  to  graft  out  unprofit- 
able ones,  comes  the  need  for  closer  attention  to  the  question  of  pollination. 
One  object  of  these  studies  is  to  determine  the  inter-fruitfulness  of  the  varieties 
that  are  now  recommended  for  planting  in  the  Annapolis  valley.  Two  other 
varieties,  viz.,  Blenheim  and  Stark,  frequently  reported  as  unfruitful,  have  also 
been  given  special  attention.  Furthermore,  it  has  been  considered  desirable  to 
make  a  thorough  study  of  insect  pollinators,  to  determine  whether  the  natural 
supply  is  sufficient  or  whether  means  should  be  taken  to  increase  their  number. 
In  this  connection  a  study  of  poisoning  among  hive  bees  as  a  result  of  orchard 
spraying  and  dusting  practices  has  been  made. 


26 

C.     CAUSES    OF    UNFRUITFULNESS 

According  to  Crane  and  Lawrence  (1929  &  1930),  unfruitfulness  in  apples  is 
associated  with  (1)  generational  sterility,  i.e.,  the  failure  of  any  of  the  processes 
concerned  in  the  normal  alternation  of  generations,  viz.,  development  of  pollen, 
embryo  sac,  embryo,  endosperm  and  the  relation  of  these  with  one  another, 
regardless  of  the  cross  made;  (2)  morphological  sterility  due  to  the  suppression 
or  abortion  of  the  sex  organs;  (3)  incompatibility ,  which  is  found  in  cases  where 
both  ovules  and  pollen  are  functional,  the  failure  to  set  fruit  being  due  to  the 
fact  that  the  pollen  tube  becomes  arrested  in  the  stylar  tissue,  whereas,  in  a 
compatible  cross,  with  the  same  pollen  and  ovules  taking  part,  the  pollen  tube 
travels  the  full  length  of  the  style,  the  male  and  female  nuclei  fuse  and  the 
fertilized  ovule  develops  into  a  fruit.  The  term  incompatibility  has  been  often 
misused  in  horticultural  literature,  being  applied  to  various  forms  of  unfruitful- 
ness in  addition  to  that  indicated;  (4)  parthenocarpy  or  the  development  of 
fruit  without  seed;    (5)   number  of  seeds  per  ovary. 

Without  embarking  upon  a  complete  discussion  of  the  genetic  constitution 
of  apple  varieties,  it  may  serve  to  clarify  future  discussion  to  refer  briefly  to 
certain  facts  in  connection  therewith.  We  owe  to  several  workers  our  present 
knowledge  of  chromosome  behaviour  in  the  apple  (vide  Kobel  1 1930,  1930a,  & 
1931),  Rybin  (1926),  Nebel  (1930),  Darlington  and  Moffet  (1930),  Moffet 
(1931),  Roscoe  (unpublished  data)).  Darlington  and  Moffet  (loc.  cit.)  show  that 
the  basic  chromosome  number  in  the  Pomoideae  is  17  and  that  diploid,  triploid 
and  tetraploid  forms  occur.  These  writers  find,  however,  that  the  chromosome 
constitution  of  apples  is  complex.  Thus  so-called  diploids  with  2n=34  are,  in 
reality,  secondary  polyploids  being  hexasomic  in  respect  to  three  chromosomes 
and  tetrasomic  in  respect  to  four  chromosomes.  Triploids  are  partly  hexasomic 
and  partly  nonasomic.  Varieties  with  intermediate  numbers  are  found,  the 
highest  frequency  in  triploid  x  diploid  seedlings  being  41.  Among  the  com- 
mercial apples  classified  as  diploids  may  be  listed  Cox  Orange,  Ben  Davis,  Spy. 
Mcintosh,  Duchess,  Red  Astrachan,  Rome  Beauty  and  Bishop  Pippin;  while 
Gravenstein,  King,  Baldwin,  Blenheim  and  R.  I.  Greening  are  triploids,  as  indi- 
cated in  the  accompanying  list.  Consideration  of  the  foregoing  facts  will  shed 
considerable  light  on  the  results  outlined  in  the  following  page-. 

TABLE  No.  3.— CHROMOSOME  NUMBERS  OF  APPLE  VARIETIES 


Variety 

Rybin 

Kobel 

Nebel 

Darlington 

and 

Moffet  t 

•M.V. 

Roscoe 

Aderslebener  Calville 

6\ 

Allington  Pippin.. . . 

34 
34 

Annie  Elizabeth 

Apfel  aus  Lunow 

34 
51 
51 

Arkansas 

Baldwin 

48-49 

51 
34 

Beauty  of  Bath 

Belle  de  Boskoop 

46 

51 
34 
51 

Ben  Davis 

Blenheim  Orange  .  . 

51 

Bobnapfel. . 

46-49 

51 

Calville  Grand  Duke  of  Asden 

34 

Carlisle  Pippin 

34 

Charlamowsky  (Duchess  of  Oldenburg). 

34 

34 

Cox  Orange  Pippin 

34 

Crimson  Beauty 

34 

Crimson  Bramley  . 

51 

Damason  Reinette 

45-47 

Deacon  Jones 

34 

Der  Boehmer 

34 

Delicious 

34 

27 

TABLE  No.  3— CHROMOSOME   NUMBERS   OF   APPLE   VARIETIES— Concluded 


Variety 

Rybin 

Kobel 

Nebel 

Darlington 
and 

Moffett 

*M.V. 

Roscoe 

Dolgo 

34 

Doucin  (Mailing  Type  II) 

34 
34 

Earlv  Victoria 

Eden 

34 
34 
34 
34 

Geheimrat  Dr.  Oldenburg 

General  von  Hammerstein 

Genet  Movie 

51 

Golden  Russet 

34 

46-46 

51 
51 

34 

34 

Horbert's  Reinette 

45 

Irish  Peach 

34 

49-51 

34 

34 

Kentish 

34 
34 

Keswick  Codlin 

Kola 

68 
34 
34 

Lane's  Prince  Albert 

34 

Lord  Derby 

34 
42 

Mcintosh 

34 
34 

Manx  Codlin 

34 

Manks  Kiichenapfel 

34 
34 
34 

Medina 

Minister  von  Hammerstein 

34 

Nonpareil  (Roxbury  Russet) 

*51 

Nonsuch  (Mailing  Type  VI) 

34 
34 
34 
34 

Northern  Spy 

Odlins 

Old  English  Broadleaf.  .  . 

Ontario 

34 

Paradise  (Mailing  Stock  Type  I) 

34 

34 
34 

Red  Siberian  Crab.... 

Red  Winter  Reinette 

34 

Reinette  du  Canada 

51 

38-40 

51 

Reinette  Zucoamaglio. 

34 
51 
34 

Ribston  Pippin  . 

42 

51 

R.I.  Greening 

51 
34 

Roter  Eiserapfel 

47 

Roter  Jumpfernapfel.  . 

34 
51 
34 
34 

Sommerrambour 

48-49 

Stark 

*51 

Tompkins  King 

51 
34 
34 

Twenty  Ounce 

Wagener 

*34 

Warner's  King 

42 

Waidners  Goldrenette 

34 

41  +  1 

34 

34 

Winter  Magetin  . 

34 

48-49 

Worcester  Pearmain     . 

34 

Wolf  River 

34 

Yellow  Bellflower 

34 
34 

*Unpublished  data,  used  by  permission. 


28 
1.  GENERATIONAL  STERILITY 

In  the  first  group  of  factors  mentioned  by  Crane  and  Lawrence  {loc.  cit.) 
as  a  cause  of  unfruitfulness,  pollen  sterility,  or  the  production  of  a  large 
percentage  of  pollen  of  low  germinability  with  a  high  percentage  of  aborted  or 
shrivelled  grains,  is  very  common.  Among  the  varieties  that,  in  our  tests,  proved 
to  have  relatively  poor  pollen,  were  Blenheim,  Baldwin,  Gravenstein  and  King. 
It  will  be  noted  that  all  these  varieties  are  triploids,  which  indicates  that  triploid 
varieties  should  not  be  depended  upon  as  male  parents,  as  all  have  given 
generally  inferior  results  over  a  period  of  years.  On  the  other  hand,  varieties 
that  give  relatively  large  amounts  of  pollen  of  good  quality,  such  as  Cox  Orange, 
Northern  Spy,  etc.,  are  diploids.  The  correlation  that  exists  between  pollen 
viability  and  chromosome  constitution  noted  by  Kobel  (1926)  is  very  evident  in 
our  tests  and  strongly  supports  the  view  that  low  pollen  germination  may  be 
considered  as  an  indication  of  triploidy.  The  low  pollen  germination  alone  does 
not  account  for  the  low  value  of  triploids  as  pollinizers.  Irregular  chromosome 
behaviour,  followed  by  abortion  in  the  embryos  is  also  a  factor.  It  may  be 
noted  that  ovule  abortion  resulting  in  a  low  average  seed  count  is  another 
characteristic  of  triploid  varieties. 

2.  MORPHOLOGICAL  STERILITY 

Detjen  (1926)  finds  that  fruits  whose  ovules  have  not  been  fertilized  are 
generally  shed  soon  after  the  blooming  period,  but  that  the  last  wave  of 
abscission,  i.e.,  the  "  July  drop  "  is  due  mainly  to  factors  which  cause  embryo 
abortion.  In  studies  of  the  Stayman  Winesap,  Howlett  (1931)  has  shown  that 
an  important  factor  in  fruit  setting  is  irregularity  in  chromosome  behaviour 
during  megasporogenesis  which  would  cause  early  dropping.  It  would  appear 
that  most  cases  of  so-called  incompatibility  are,  in  reality,  cases  of  embryo 
abortion  due  to  irregular  chromosome  distribution. 

3.    INCOMPATIBILITY 

As  stated  by  Crane  and  Lawrence  (1930),  among  cultivated  apples,  sterility 
and  incompatibility  differ  in  several  respects  from  the  same  phenomena  in 
plums  and  cherries.  A  salient  difference  is  that  incompatibility  is  rarely,  if  ever, 
completely  expressed  in  apples.  These  workers  are  writing  from  a  breeding 
standpoint  and  with  special  reference  to  the  production  of  viable  seed,  but  this 
fact  also  applies,  to  a  certain  degree,  to  fruit  fulness.  A  high  degree  of  self- 
incompatibility  has  been  claimed  for  many  varieties,  including  Gravenstein,  Spy, 
Cox  Orange  and  Blenheim.  With  such  varieties,  even  when  the  pollen  is  removed 
from  the  anthers  and  matured  under  ideal  conditions,  it  practically  never 
produces  a  satisfactory  set  of  fruit  in  self-pollination  tests.  At  the  other  extreme 
is  Baldwin,  which,  though  producing  pollen  of  low  value  for  crossing  purposes, 
may  yield  satisfactory  crops  when  selfed,  though  not  as  large  crops  as  when 
pollinated  with  pollen  from  a  diploid  variety. 

Cross-incompatibility  has  also  been  claimed  for  various  combinations  of 
apple  varieties,  though  some  workers  question  this.  Howlett  (1927)  in  experi- 
ments in  Ohio,  found  no  evidence  of  cross-incompatibility  and  considered 
apparent  oases  of  this  phenomenon  to  be  examples  of  "  cross-sterility,  inter- 
sexualism,  due  to  impotence  of  pollen."  Einset  (1930)  tested  a  number  of  eases 
of  alleged  cross-incompatibility.  Only  in  the  case  of  Arkansas  and  Grimes  was 
there  indication  of  true  cross-incompatibility  in  the  sense  employed  by  East  and 
Manglesdorf  (1925),  and  Crane  and  Lawrence  (1929).  The  remainder  of  their 
tests  showed  not  incompatibility  but  sterility,  which  they  consider  to  be  due 
to  the  effects  of  zygotic  abortion  caused  by  irregularities  in  the  reduction 
division  in  the  generative  cells. 


29 

4.     PARTHENOCARPY 

Several  varieties  of  apples  have  been  reported  as  showing  a  tendency  to  set 
fruit  with  few  or  no  seeds.  Gravenstein  is  the  most  important  of  our  local 
varieties  to  ©how  this  tendency.  Ribston  Pippin,  not  studied  by  us,  is  another. 
Among  other  reported  varieties  are  Antonovka,  Alexander,  Crimson  Bramley 
and  Longfield.  Kobel  (1930  &  1931)  noted  the  tendency  of  varieties  with 
low  pollen  germination  and  irregular  chromosome  distribution  to  set  few  or  no 
seeds.  He  designates  as  "  false  parthenocarpy,"  those  cases  in  which  fertiliza- 
tion has  actually  taken  place,  but,  owing  to  the  irregular  chromosome  distribu- 
tion, seed  development  does  not  take  place.  Even  in  Gravenstein,  which  shows 
the  greatest  tendency  to  produce  seedless  or  few  seeded  fruit,  there  is  strong 
evidence  that  seed  formation  is  a  decided  advantage  in  fruit  setting,  and  Einset 
(1930)  has  even  found  a  correlation  between  seed  content  and  weight,  though 
such  a  correlation  was  not  obtained  by  us. 

Extensive  counts  from  various  standard  varieties  always  reveal  a  higher 
average  seed  count  in  apples  that  remain  on  the  tree  than  in  those  that  come  off 
in  the  July  drop.  Individual  fruits  may  vary,  but  the  foregoing  was  true  for 
averages.  Numerous  workers  have  held  that  such  production  is  so  intimately 
associated  with  the  physiological  processes  of  the  fruit,  that  apples  with 
developed  seeds  have  an  advantage  in  the  competition  for  water  and  organic 
nutrients  over  those  that  have  fewer  or  no  seeds  and  this  is  confirmed  by  our 
results.  It  is,  however,  true  that  triploid  varieties  are  able  to  set  fruit  with  a 
lower  content  of  developed  seed  than  in  the  case  in  diploids.  Data  with  respect 
to  the  relation  between  fruitfulness  and  the  production  of  seeds  and  seedlings 
are  presented  later. 

In  connection  with  the  production  of  seedlings  resulting  from  various  crosses, 
the  difference  between  diploids  and  triploids  is  very  pronounced,  the  latter,  as  a 
group,  giving  a  very  low  percentage  of  seedlings.  The  number  of  seeds  and 
seedlings  resulting  from  different  crosses  on  any  given  female  parent  should 
therefore,  be  a  useful  index  of  the  value  of  the  male  parent  as  a  pollinizer  for 
that  variety.  But  within  the  triploid  group  those  crosses  that  give  the  larger 
number  of  seed  are  more  fruitful  than  those  that  yield  a  smaller  number,  though 
the  average  number  of  seed  for  the  group  is  much  less  than  in  diploids.  Data 
bearing  on  this  point  are  presented  in  another  section. 

D.  PERCENTAGE  OF  FRUIT  TO  FLOWERS  REQUIRED  TO  GIVE 

AN    ECONOMIC    YIELD 

This  problem  is  discussed  only  from  the  standpoint  of  commercial  orchards 
in  the  Annapolis  valley.  As  previously  noted  the  per  cent  of  blossoms,  which, 
in  the  apple,  is  required  to  develop  into  mature  fruit  is  small  compared  with 
stone  fruits.  The  percentage  that  actually  sets  fruit  varies  with  different  factors 
already  discussed,  according  to  the  amount  of  bloom  present  on  the  tree,  and 
with  the  variety.  With  a  small  bloom,  as  many  as  fifteen,  twenty  or  even 
a  higher  percentage  may  be  obtained;  but,  with  a  full  bloom,  five  per  cent  is 
the  figure  ordinarily  given  as  representing  a  satisfactory  commercial  set.  This 
is  a  convenient,  though  arbitrary  figure,  since,  of  course,  varieties  differ  in 
regard  to  the  number  of  apples  necessary  to  produce  a  crop  that  can  be  called 
commercial.  A  number  of  workers  (vide  MacDaniels,  1930)  stress  the  advis- 
ability of  taking  into  consideration  the  number  of  fruiting  centres.  Howlett 
(1929),  for  example,  finds  that  in  Stayman  Winesap  one  apple  to  every  third 
flowering  spur  is  sufficient  for  a  commercial  crop.  Lattimer  (1931)  contends 
that  more  attention  should  be  given  to  expressing  fruit-set  on  the  basis  of 
spurs  rather  than  individual  flowers,  pointing  out  that  if  more  than  one  fruit 
per  spur  sets  it  should  be  thinned.  The  foregoing  is  undoubtedly  true  and, 
under  orchard  conditions,  it  is  often  advisable  to  thin  a  tree  that  is  bearing 
less  than   a  normal   crop,   because   of   clustering   on    a   portion   of  the   spurs. 


30 


Nevertheless,   where   natural   pollination   occurs   there   is   no   way   of   avoiding 
uneven  setting. 

Some  varieties  are  poor  female  parents,  i.e.,  they  shed  a  higher  proportion 
of  blossom  and  fruit  than  others,  having  a  tendency  to  thin  down  to  one  fruit 
to  the  spur;  others  have  a  tendency  to  set  in  clusters.  The  fruiting  habits 
of  a  number  of  standard  varieties  are  shown  in  the  accompanying  table,  in 
which  the  year  1929  is  taken  as  the  basis  for  comparison  with  the  three-year 
average  for  1928-1930  inclusive.  In  this  year  all  trees  blossomed  heavily  and 
the  set  was  unusually  good. 

TABLE  No.  4.— BLOSSOMING  AND  FRUITING  HABITS  OF  FOUR  STANDARD  VARIETIES 

(Average  of  16  Orchards) 


Variety 


Per  cent 
fruit 
(1929) 


Crop  per 

tree  1929 

(bbls.) 


Average 

crop 
in  bbls. 

(1928-30) 


Average 

number 

apples  per 

tree 

(1929) 


Average       Calcu- 
number         lated. 
apples  per    number 
bbl.         blossoms 
(1929)        per  tree 
(1929) 


Gravenstein 

King 

Baldwin 

Spy 


5-68 

4-49 

11-32 

9-13 


2-67 
4-10 
3-73 
4-11 


3  00 
3-02 
501 
4-26 


1,291 
1,640 
2,051 
2,053 


430 
543 
681 

482 


22,720 
36,525 
18,122 
22,508 


From  these  figures  it  will  be  seen  that  though  the  King  trees  included 
in  this  survey  have  a  larger  average  number  of  blossoms  than  the  other  varieties, 
the  percentage  of  fruit  is  lower.  Nevertheless,  since  the  King  apples  are  large. 
the  final  number  of  barrels  produced  is  short  only  of  the  Spies.  In  the  case 
of  Baldwin,  which  has  a  decided  tendency  to  set  in  clusters,  the  percentage  set 
is  heaviest,  but,  since  the  number  of  blossoms  per  tree  is  the  smallest  and  the 
fruit  size  is  also  the  smallest,  the  total  crop  in  barrels  is  less  than  that  of 
King  or  Spy.  The  heavier  set  obtained  with  this  variety  may  be  a  factor  in 
its  biennial  bearing  habit  in  so  many  orchards. 

Comparing  the  foregoing  figures  with  the  average  result  of  hand  pollina- 
tions carried  out  for  the  three-year  period  1928-1930,  we  find  that  Baldwin 
has  given  an  average  per  cent  fruit  of  11-07;  Cox  Orange,  7-64;  Golden  Russet. 
4-40;  Gravenstein,  6-16;  King,  4-0;  Spy,  7-63.  The  massed  results  showing 
average  figures  for  the  four  varieties  from  16  commercial  orchards  may  be  of 
some  interest  in  this  connection  and  are  given   for  comparison. 


TABLE   No.  5.— FRUITING   HABITS  OF  FOUR  STANDARD   VARIETIES, 

FOUR  FOR  1928-1930 


AVERAGE  OF 


Orchard 

Total 
blooming 

spurs 
counted 

Average 

per  cctn 
with 
bloom 

Average 
blossoms 

per 

blooming 
spur 

Average 

per  cent 
fruit 

A 

3,  54(5 
2,870 
3,439 
2,031 
3,089 
2.094 
3.096 
2,861 
2,619 
2.S5I 
2,541 
2,558 
3.344 
3,252 
3,009 
3,329 

55-50 

44-25 
44-74 
34-51 
44-69 
62  03 
42-97 
54-13 
47-32 
60-69 
73  16 
59-39 
55-81 
54-68 
.-,1-ss 

49-4S 

508 
5  07 
5   17 
5-39 
4-90 
4-83 
4  ■  75 
4-63 
4-91 
4-70 
4-85 
502 
4-64 
4-89 
4  70 
4-56 

11-44 

B 

9-79 

c 

6-23 

CA 

7-26 

D 

2  07 

E 

F 

S01 

6-64 

G 

6-46 

H 

I ; .... 

731 
5-76 

K 

6-49 

L 

10-59 

M 

8-74 

N 

7-19 

O 

B- 17 

P 

III.  EXPERIMENTAL  STUDIES  IN  APPLE 
POLLINATION 

W.    H.    BRITTAIN    and    DONALD    S.    BLAIR 


A.    CONDITIONS    FOR    CROSS-POLLINATION 

In  considering  the  pollination  question  for  commercial  orchards  two 
questions  must  be  kept  in  mind,  viz.,  (1)  the  necessity  of  an  abundant  supply 
of  effective  pollinizers  for  each  variety  planted  in  the  orchard  and  (2)  the 
presence  of  an  adequate  force  of  pollinating  insects  to  insure  that  cross-pollina- 
tion is  effected. 

The  conditions  that  may  be  found  within  a  commercial  planting  are  as 
follows: — 

1.  The  orchard  may  be  planted  with  a  suitable  mixture  of  cross-fruitful 
varieties. 

2.  The  orchard  may  be  set  out  with  cross-unfruitful  varieties,  e.g.,  Graven- 
stein,  Blenheim  and  Stark  planted  together. 

3.  Varieties  may  be  planted  together  in  such  a  way  that  one  variety  is 
not  supplied  with  an  effective  pollinizer,  e.g.,  Cox  Orange  planted  with  Blen- 
heim. In  this  case  Cox  Orange  is  a  suitable  pollinizer  for  Blenheim,  but  the 
Blenheim  (an  ineffective  pollinizer)  is  incapable  of  cross-pollinating  the  self- 
unfruitful  Cox  Orange. 

4.  Self-unfruitful  varieties  may  be  planted  in  such  large  blocks  that 
cross-pollination  is  impossible. 

5.  For  any  one  of  the  above  conditions  an  effective  population  of  pollinators 
may  be  lacking,  which  would  be  a  limiting  factor  in  each  case. 

B.    PREVIOUS    WORK    WITH    BEES    AS    POLLINATORS    UNDER 
CONTROLLED  CONDITIONS 

Several  workers  have  tested  the  value  of  bees  in  experiments  in  which 
hives  are  introduced  beneath  tents  covering  the  trees.  In  some  of  these  cases  the 
necessity  for  cross-pollination  is  apparently  ignored  as  no  source  of  compatible 
pollen  was  supplied,  resulting  in  selfing  and  a  low  set  of  fruit,  as  might  be 
expected.  Morris  (1921)  found  no  advantage  from  having  Jonathan  and  Rome 
Beauty  trees  selfed  by  bees.  Macoun  (1923)  found  that  Mcintosh  set  a  satis- 
factory crop  when  supplied  with  bees  and  a  source  of  compatible  pollen  or 
when  wild  bees  were  allowed  access  to  the  flowers  under  the  same  conditions, 
but  a  very  low  yield  was  obtained  when  all  bees  were  excluded.  Hutson  (1926) 
tented  two  trees  each  of  Wealthy  and  Jonathan,  one  each  with  bees  and  one 
without.  The  Wealthy  gave  a  set  of  17  per  cent  with  bees  and  4-02  per  cent 
without;  the  Jonathan  gave  8-4  per  cent  with  bees  and  -80  without.  Many 
workers  have  noted  that  caged  trees  or  bagged  limbs  from  which  bees  are 
excluded  yield  little  or  no  fruit  and  much  similar  information  is  available  from 
experiments  in  inter-fruitfulness  of  varieties  carried  out  under  tents,  to  which 
reference  is  made  elsewhere. 

31 


32 

C.  EXPERIMENTS  IN  BEES  AND  POLLINATION 

(Tent  Studies) 

1.  OUTLINE   OF   EXPERIMENTS 

In  order  to  determine  the  effect  of  the  different  treatments  outlined  under 
the  heading  "  Conditions  for  Cross-pollination,"  there  was  initiated  in  1929  a 
series  of  experiments  in  which  bearing  trees  of  four  standard  varieties  were 
enclosed  in  tents  and  subjected  to  conditions  in  simulation  of  those  referred  to  in 
the  foregoing.  The  trees  then  were  seventeen  years  old,  in  excellent  condition 
and  grown  under  similar  conditions  of  culture.  All  these  experiments  were 
carried  on  at  the  Experimental  Station  orchard  at  Kentville. 

The  following  were  the  treatments  accorded  to  the  trees  in  the  different 
tents : 

(a)  Supplied  with  pollinizing  bouquets  of  an  effective  pollinizer  and  with 
a  colony  of  bees,  thus  furnishing  the  tree  both  with  a  proper  pollen  supply  and 
with  insect  pollinators. 

(6)  Supplied  with  pollinizing  bouquets  of  an  ineffective  pollinizer  arid  with 
a  colony  of  bees,  thus  furnishing  the  tree  with  adequate  provision  for  cross- 
pollination,  but  with  a  supply  of  unsuitable  pollen. 

(c)  Supplied  with  a  colony  of  bees,  but  with  no  pollinizing  bouquets  of  any 
kind,  thus  ensuring  that  the  tree  would  be  self-pollinated  by  the  bees. 

(d)  Supplied  with  neither  bees  nor  bouquets  of  any  kind,  thus  preventing 
pollination  except  such  self-pollination  as  would  occur  through  the  agency  of 
wind  or  gravity. 

(e)  Supplied  with  bouquets  of  an  effective  pollinizer  but  with  no  bees. 
This  test  was  started  in  1930.  This  tree,  therefore,  while  having  a  supply  of 
suitable  pollen  provided,  was  deprived  of  the  normal  agency  of  cross-pollination, 
except  in  so  far  as  wind  may  be  effective  in  this  regard.  In  1931  and  1932  a 
current  of  air  from  an  orchard  duster  was  blown  very  thoroughly  through  the 
bouquets  and  over  the  trees.  It  was  only  in  1932  that  there  was  any  evidence 
that  any  appreciable  pollination  was  achieved  by  this  method.  This  is 
accounted  for  by  improvement  in  technique  in  the  latter  year.  About  half  an 
hour  was  taken  at  each  tree  and  the  air  current  directed  through  the  bouquet 
in  all  directions.  It  is  highly  improbable,  however,  that  ordinary  wind  could 
produce  the  same  effect  as  a  current  of  air  produced  in  the  foregoing  manner. 

(/)  The  results  from  the  foregoing  trees  were  compared  with  records  from 
neighbouring  untented  trees  grown  under  similar  conditions.  Since  the  orchard 
was  well  laid  out  from  the  standpoint  of  the  intermixture  of  cross-fruitful 
varieties  and,  since  colonies  of  bees  were  placed  throughout  the  orchard,  this 
series  was  provided  both  with  an  adequate  supply  of  suitable  pollen  and  with 
insect  pollinators  sufficient  to  ensure  cross-pollination.  In  1931,  owing  to  a 
certain  amount  of  poisoning  among  the  bees  and  probably  other  factors,  the 
bee  population  was  lower  than  in  other  seasons. 

As  a  further  check  on  the  foregoing  experiments  three  limbs  of  as  nearly 
as  possible  uniform  size  and  condition  were  selected  in  each  tent  and  treated 
as  follows: 

1.  On  trees  with  bees  and  bouquets,  (i.e.  on  trees  (a)  and  (b)): 

(i)   Enclosed  one  limb  with  a  cheesecloth  bag  and  allowed   it   to  remain 

untouched. 
(ii)  Bagged  one  limb  and  selj '-pollinated  the  blossoms. 
(iii)  Bagged  one  limb  and  pollinated  blossoms,  with   the  same  variety  of 

pollen  used  in  the  bouquets — an  effective  pollinizer  in   (a)   treatment 

and  an  ineffective  pollinizer  in  (b)  treatment. 


33 


60796—3 


34 

2.  On  trees  with  no  bouquets  {i.e.  (c)  and  (d)  and  trees  with  no  bees 
(i.e.  (e) )  and  on  the  untented  tree  {i.e.  (/) ) : 

(i)  Bagged  one  limb  and  left  it  untouched. 
(ii)  Bagged  one   limb  and  self -pollinated   blossoms. 

(iii)  Bagged  one  limb  and  pollinated  the  blossoms  ivith  pollen  from  a  cross- 
fruitful  variety. 

Thus,  in  each  case  we  have  for  comparison  individual  hand  pollinated  or 
untouched  limbs  to  check  against  the  results  from  pollination  with  bees,  or 
with  blossoms  unpollinated  except  by  wind. 

The  varieties  chosen  for  these  tests  were  Gravenstein,  King,  Baldwin, 
and  Spy,  a  range  covering  the  blossoming  season.  Included  are  two  varieties 
which  may  be  numbered  among  the  most  self-unfruitful  of  commercial  sorts, 
viz.,  Gravenstein  and  Spy.  The  former  is  a  triploid  form  having  a  tendency 
to  produce  a  certain  number  of  seedless  or  few-seeded  fruits,  especially  under 
some  conditions.  Intermediate  in  position  is  the  King,  which,  in  some  seasons, 
has  appeared  as  self-unfruitful  as  any  variety,  but  in  others  has  given  a  fair 
set  when  selfed,  though  never  as  large  a  set  as  when  crossed  with  a  cross-fruitful 
variety.  Baldwin  is  a  striking  exception  in  that  it  is  uniformly  self-fruitful 
to  a  larger  extent  than  any  variety  tested.  This  variety,  though  a  triploid 
form,  with  pollen  of  low  germinability  and  possessing  a  high  proportion 
of  aborted  grains,  very  inferior  for  crossing,  is  yet  highly  self-compatible. 
Gravenstein,  though  varying  considerably  from  year  to  year  is  markedly 
inferior  for  crossing  purposes  to  such  varieties  as  Cox  Orange,  Wagener,  Golden 
Russet,  etc.,  which,  with  Northern  Spy,  are  among  the  most  effective  pollinizers 
among  commercial  sorts  grown  in  the  Annapolis  valley. 

For  pollinizing  bouquets,  limbs  of  the  desired  varieties  well  covered  with 
bloom  were  removed  and  placed  in  large  cans  of  water.  The  first  year,  only  a 
single  bouquet  was  used  per  tent,  but  it  was  found  that  the  set  on  the  side 
next  the  bouquet  was  much  greater  than  that  on  the  opposite  side.  Therefore,  in 
following  years  two  bouquets,  one  on  each  side  of  the  tree,  were  employed.  In 
the  very  hot  blossoming  season  of  1930  there  was  a  decided  tendency  to  wilt 
on  the  part  of  the  bouquets,  which  made  them  less  attractive  to  the  bees  and 
had  an  adverse  effect  on  setting.  This  was  avoided  in  1931  by  frequent  chang- 
ing of  the  bouquets  whenever  it  became  necessary. 

For  ineffective  pollinizers  Blenheim  was  generally  used  for  Gravenstein 
and  King.  For  Baldwin  and  Spy,  Nonpareil  was  employed.  As  effective  pol- 
linizers we  used  Wagener  for  Gravenstein,  Wagener  or  Golden  Russet  for  King, 
Cox  Orange  for  Baldwin  and  the  same  variety  or  Ben  Davis  for  Spy. 

Package  colonies  of  bees  were  used  as  pollinators  and  in  1928-1931  proved 
quite   satisfactory.     In    1932   overwintered   colonies   were   used. 

The  results  of  tent  experiments  have  been  discounted  by  certain  workers 
on  account  of  the  supposedly  "  unnatural  conditions  "  but  we  were  unable  to 
secure  any  evidence  to  indicate  that  our  results  were  affected  by  artificial  con- 
ditions inside  the  tent.  Hygrograph  and  thermograph  charts  herein  presented 
show  a  remarkably  slight  variation  between  inside  and  outside  condition-. 

2.    GENERAL    RESULTS    OF    TENT    EXPERIMENTS 

The  accompanying  table  summarizes  the  results  obtained  from  this  Beries. 
These  results  bring  out  clearly  the  necessity  of  proper  attention  being  given 
to  the  right  admixture  of  varieties  in  orchard  plantings  and  equally  the  impor- 
tance of  the  presence  of  pollinating  insects  in  order  to  obtain  a  commercial 
crop. 

All  varieties  respond  by  increased  sets  of  fruit  to  the  application  of  the 
pollen  by   cross-fruitful  varieties  to   their   stigmas   and   all   bnt    Baldwin   give 


35 

decidedly  inferior  results  when  selfed.  This  is  less  true  of  King  than  the  other 
varieties  tested,  which,  from  the  standpoint  of  per  cent  fruit  obtained,  may  be 
considered  a  poor  female  parent,  and  shows  less  difference  than  other  varieties 
between  effective  and  ineffective  male  parents.  The  abundant  bloom  of  this 
variety,  coupled  with  the  large  size  of  the  fruit  and  its  annual  bearing  habits 
compensates,  in  a  measure,  for  the  low  percentage  fruit  obtained  with  all 
crosses,  so  that  King  may  be  regarded  as  a  commercially  fruitful  variety. 


■=*TtT 

r~"M  (\rf\  Ul/f     /  H\ffXfl\^Tl 

V 

\       U     V            w                     \i     V      V       v 

1       1 

i       \       \       \       \       \       \       \       \       \  v**"**"'\     . 

/?aifS4-Jur7e  6  /ffJ/ 


/VaY&  —  June  9.  /S3& 


Sfay34  -<Jvr?e3.  /&J/ 


/Yai/  S8  —  dv/?e&.  /9SQ 


Fig.  4. — Humidity  and  temperatin 

r   " 

June  9,  1932    (original) 


we  record®  inside  and  outside  tents,  Kentville,  N.S.      (1) 
24- June  6,  ;    (2)  May  24- June  3,  1931;    (3)  May  28- June  9,  1932;    (4)   May  28- 


The  average  seed  per  fruit  for  each  treatment  in  tent  series  was  taken 
in  1932.  This  figure  proved  to  be  a  very  reliable  index  as  to  fruitfulness.  For 
each  variety  tested,  either  diploid  or  triploid,  the  seed  content  per  apple 
increased  proportionally  to  the  pollination  provided,  i.e.,  the  more  perfect  the 
conditions  for  pollination  the  greater  the  number  of  seeds  per  fruit.  The  1932 
tented  figures  are  in  agreement  with  those  obtained  in  the  standard  varieties, 
during  the  period  1928-1932. 

In  order  to  avoid  repetition  a  discussion  of  the  detailed  results  of  these 
experiments  is  included  in  the  next  section. 

60796— 3 1 


36 


II 


««  O 


ft  gg 


ft  o  in 


^8 


^1 

^3  S 


£-2§ 


111 


£j6 
^3  S 


SB 


,6  8  6 


MNtD*03>         OlCNOOiMn         NcOMUlTticO        f-  O  CM 
r-  CM  f  -h  CO  tf         COiO-HNMCO         OliOtOiHON         MNCN 

^HHHNW         (ONMMCOlC        «5  CM  CM  »-h  CO  »0         CO  ~H  CM 


(N-HCDCJ         CO 


OiONcj 


r—  ob  oi  as      oo 


t^  en  co  co      f 
»o  as  cx3  o      «o 

T    -  T)  3  O 


OOOOO^        ~< 


MflNUJOl' 


O  O  O  CO  CM  O 


CO  —  no  CO    —    ~. 
OOCNCO'tCNiO 

OrHHOOrt 


(NNOCON         t-  CO  O  O  I 

iOO>CO-!f  O         — I  t-  — .  CO  I 

OCJCOOJN         -h  CM  «  CO  C 


■-<  -h  CO  ■*  CM  »0  >— i  33  O0  33  00  CM  Tf -h  to  "t  00  O  lOOiMsOiffM 
rt!0OW*O  CO  CO  f  CM  1^-  33  lO  00  00  iO -«J*  CO  O0  CO  —  ■>»•  Tf  oo 
00  CO  CD  "CP  CO  O         CO  f  lO  >0  CM  CO         MCiO"*tN         Ot»OOlOKHO 


lOOUJi'OCO 

o  o  o  o  o  o 


■  CO«CCOCO         C0OOO00N 
I  O  O  O  —I         -h  CM  -h  CO  CO  CO 


O  O]  TO  O  "*T  CO 

O  CM  —  —  O  •"»" 

oooo— o 


o  o  f  co  o  r-~      03  —  - 


>  O--     - 

IO  <--■  CO  oo 

iOhOON        f<J>  CO  "<f  f  CO 


03  3:  'O  32  co  •'J'        t^  f  O  CO  O  O 
O   —  CO  CO  CO  o 


t~-  00  t^  O  CM  CO 


Of  CNNt^-i  CM  O0  CM  CO  —  f  r^  CO  t^  O  «5  o  «0  O  ©  "0  O  CO 

OlrirtCOtOl  VlOMOXN  -iCiNTTOI"  onooocnoo 

CNOOO)  "5  CO  CO  «— i  — i  f  MifNCJiOO  O  CM  CM  O  —  00 

00  — i  »C  O  33  CM  «-"  >C  f  CO  "2  CM  -*  CO  CO  CO  t~-  CO  —  f  O  33  CO  t— 

MNOOICOCO  ~  ©  iO  —  CO  f  —  uo  CO  CM  CO  CM  CNOVOOON 

hhmooh  o  — "Of  oho  -cmxaio  as  co  cm  -*  —  o 

CM           CM  CO  CM  <— i      >-«  CO  CM  CM      CO  f           CM 

-<f  CM  00  CO  CM  CO  t^  00  lO  33  00  CM  »  CO  CM  CM  CO  >C  — J  33  CO  CO  CO  CO  r- 

«OtO-HrtCTi  fCMCOfOOO  3CCIt-X  00  —  O  33  CO  00 

00  lO  t^  CO  33  CO  cjNMNXr-  V  35  ■*  Ol  N  CO  f  CO  ©  CO  ©  33 

o'  o"  —  O*  ■»*>"  cm'  ddoOxf  o"  o"  co"  t~-  33  f  33  t^-~  o"  o"  o"  CO  Oi 


r 

si 


-.    >    3    O  *^ 


£.fi 


l«S 


w   eg   ro   03   qj   qj   — 
g  n  en  ob-O^  C    , 


IS" 


J 


12121:  x  <n"o  I 
c  c  C  Oj  o  x    I 

«  M  0j  4)  0)  aC 
en  x  cn-C-Q  C'C 
0>   03   0)    -,    -,   4)    > 


J2    0)    o    3. 

"to-Q  c  °  * 

-  Mail! 

w  "w  "O     r«     cn  T?  ^  "^  "^     CD    CC  "^ 

ccc££2       c  C  C  zi  t  2 

s  i  855IJ  I  sl^o^l 


3  S— 

O  x  ^ 


4)    >>  jj 

SJ=   O 




37 


D.  STUDIES  IN  THE  INTER-FRUITFULNESS  OF  STANDARD 

VARIETIES 


1.    OBJECT    OF    EXPERIMENTS 

The  fact  has  been  brought  out  in  the  preceding  experiments  that  all  varieties 
benefit  by  cross-pollination.  Furthermore,  it  is  clear  that  it  is  not  sufficient 
merely  to  plant  together  varieties  that  blossom  at  the  same  time,  since  some 
varieties  give  poor  results  when  used  to  pollinate  others.  Some  that  are  good 
pollinizers  for  other  varieties  are  themselves  highly  self-unfruitful  and  some, 
that  yield  good  results  when  pollinated  with  pollen  from  a  suitable  variety,  are 
themselves  deficient  as  pollinizers.  A  study  of  the  results  obtained  in  hand- 
pollination  tests  gives  useful  information  as  to  the  inter-fruitfulness  of  the 
varieties  tested  and  also  makes  clear  certain  general  principles  applicable  to  all 
work  of  this  kind. 

The  varieties  chosen  for  these  standard  tests  were  as  follows: — 

Gravenstein  Baldwin  i 

King  Cox  Orange  i 

Golden  Russet  Northern  Spy 

Certain  other  varieties  were  used  as  pollinizers  as  opportunity  offered. 
Mcintosh  might  well  have  been  added  to  the  list,  but  so  much  information  is 
available  regarding  this  variety  from  other  sources  that  it  was  not  considered 
necessary.  Special  tests  with  the  varieties  Blenheim  and  Stark  are  described  in 
another  section. 

2.   TECHNIQUE   OF   PROCEDURE   AND    DEFINITIONS   OF   TERMS   USED 

Vigorous  trees  of  the  same  age  and  growing  under  similar  cultural  conditions 
at  the  Experimental  Station,  Kentville,  were  selected.  In  a  few  cases,  where 
the  desired  variety  did  not  happen  to  be  available  through  lack  of  sufficient 
bloom,  resort  to  other  orchards  was  made,  since  in  all  cases,  only  trees  in  good 
bloom  were  used. 

During  the  first  year  of  our  work  as  many  crosses  as  possible  were  placed 
on  a  single  limb,  in  order  to  give  each  cross  an  even  chance  with  others.    In  1929, 


■/  /  / 

/ 

/ 

/ 

/ 

/ 

/j/V/i//va//v/\///: 

a  ~\  J  ~ 

A 

.  H 

•A 

A 

/w  v  yj   v  r  v  °    n 

3D                           1 

si 

sy 

\ 

J 

I            1                                        » 

Sfai/  26  -  June  9.  /AX? 


Sfai/23-June//.  /028 
Fig.  5. — Thermograph   records  during  blooming  period,    1928-30,  Kentville,   N.S.    (original). 

however,  six  trees  of  each  variety  were  selected  and  in  1930-1932,  ten  trees.  A 
different  limb  was  selected  on  each,  for  each  cross  that  it  was  desired  to  make. 
In  order  to  reduce  to  a  minimum  any  error  resulting  from  differences  in  limbs 
from  different  sides  of  the  tree  a  rotation  of  the  different  hand  pollinated  limbs 
was  practised.    This  method  was  carried  out  throughout  the  experiment. 


38 


— 

o 
M 

g3 

e 


p. 


39 

Owing  to  nutritional  variation  between  trees  and  even  between  limbs  on 
the  same  tree,  a  certain  number  of  laterals  in  all  limbs  used  in  the  crossing  work 
were  selected  to  be  self-pollinated,  to  establish  a  uniform  basis  for  comparison 
and  to  reduce  error  to  a  minimum.  Owing  to  the  fact  that  this  method  tended 
to  complicate  record  taking  this  practice  was  discontinued  in  1932  and  individual 
limbs  were  also  used  for  selfing  tests. 

During  the  first  year  paper  bags  were  used,  but  it  was  found  much  more 
convenient  and  equally  effective  to  use  cheese  cloth  bags  covering  the  entire 
limb.  The  large  sets  obtained  from  suitable  crosses  under  these  conditions  do 
not  indicate  that  the  normal  process  of  pollination  and  fertilization  was  inter- 
fered with  by  this  treatment. 

When  the  majority  of  stigmas  for  any  variety  were  receptive  they  were 
brushed  with  the  appropriate  pollen,  applied  by  means  of  a  camel's  hair  brush, 
all  blossoms  that  had  not  reached  or  had  passed  the  receptive  stage  being  removed. 
After  the  stigmas  commenced  to  wither  the  bags  were  taken  off. 

In  these  studies  emasculation  was  neglected  in  order  to  eliminate  work  and 
to  make  it  possible  to  perform  a  very  large  number  of  pollinations.  A  word  of 
explanation  will  make  clear  the  reason  for  this  practice.  The  object  of  these 
investigations  was  not  to  secure  seed  from  various  crosses  for  breeding  work, 
but  to  test  the  value  of  the  pollen  of  one  variety  as  a  means  of  improving  the 
set  of  the  variety  considered  as  a  female  parent.  As  tests  of  self-fruitfulness 
were  conducted  in  the  manner  already  described,  the  per  cent  of  self-fruitfulness 
obtained  may  be  compared  with  the  result  of  each  cross  on  each  limb.  While 
emasculation  might  provide  better  control,  as  far  as  contamination  from  its  own 
pollen  is  concerned,  it  reduces  the  number  of  pollinations  possible  and  provides 
a  further  source  of  error  due  to  emasculation  injury. 

The  low,  and,  in  many  cases,  almost  negligible  set  obtained  from  most  self- 
pollinations  under  bags,  further  reduces  the  chances  of  error  and,  when  it  is 
considered  that  all  pollinations  were  made  with  previously  prepared  pollen  applied 
to  the  stigma  of  the  flower  as  far  as  possible  before  their  own  anthers  had 
dehisced,  it  will  be  seen  that  this  source  of  error  is  greatly  reduced.  Further- 
more, a  study  of  the  characteristics  of  seedlings  obtained  from  the  seed  saved 
from  the  various  crosses,  indicates  that  selfing  could  not  have  taken  place  to 
such  an  extent  as  to  prevent  the  results  of  these  tests  from  being  very  significant. 
In  the  case  of  Baldwin  considerable  selfing  undoubtedly  occurred,  this  variety 
showing  a  high  degree  of  self-fruitfulness  in  all  years,  but,  this  fact  being  clearly 
recognized,  it  does  not  affect  the  nature  of  the  results  from  a  pollination 
standpoint. 

Our  conclusions  with  respect  to  the  different  crosses  are  based  upon  a 
consideration  of  (1)  the  count  of  fruit  as  obtained  after  the  unfertilized  blossoms 
have  been  shed,  and  (2)  the  percentage  fruit  that  remains  after  the  "July  drop" 
and  (3)  upon  the  fruit  obtained  at  picking  time.  The  term  "set,"  as  used  in 
this  report,  applies  to  the  first  of  these,  i.e.  the  count  made  before  the  "July 
drop."  This,  by  some  workers,  is  considered  the  best  criterion  of  the  value  of 
different  crosses,  as  they  consider  that  nutritional  factors  play  a  large  part  in 
the  later  drop.  The  term  "per  cent  fruit,"  as  used  in  this  report,  refers  to  the 
percentage  fruit  that  remains  after  the  "July  drop." 

After  a  careful  study  of  all  relevant  data  it  would  appear  that  this  figure 
affords  the  best  index  of  cross-fruitfulness  or  unfruitfulness.  In  the  case  of  some 
varieties  there  is  little  difference  between  the  "per  cent  set"  and  "per  cent 
fruit."  Gravenstein  may  be  cited  as  an  example  of  such  a  variety.  In  other 
cases  the  original  set  for  all  male  parents  may  be  much  the  same,  but,  after  the 
"July  drop,"  a  wide  difference  becomes  apparent  between  diploid  and  triploid 
male  parents.  An  example  of  the  foregoing  is  Stark.  The  percentage  that 
remains  on  the  tree  at  picking  time  is  usually  little  different,  except  for  abnormal 


40 

conditions,  or  for  purely  accidental  causes,  as  high  winds,  which,  when  they  occur, 
considerably  reduce  the  value  of  this  figure.  Another  point  that  has  been  con- 
sidered in  weighing  the  value  of  various  crosses  is  the  question  of  seed  production. 
The  term  "per  cent  seed"  refers  to  the  seed  produced  from  the  original  number 
of  blossoms  pollinated  and  the  term  "per  cent  seedlings"  refers  to  seedlings 
produced  from  the  original  number  of  blossoms  pollinated.  The  average  num- 
ber of  seed  per  fruit  was  also  found  to  be  significant. 

In  these  experiments  we  have  not  recorded  the  number  of  fruits  borne  on 
a  spur.  While  it  might  doubtless  be  advantageous  to  take  into  account  the 
number  of  fruiting  centres  in  expressing  results,  the  large  number  of  pollina- 
tions made,  the  fact  that  only  normally  blossoming  limbs  were  selected,  and  the 
length  of  time  over  which  the  work  has  been  carried  out,  are  considered  to  have 
evened  out  spur  differences  and  to  give  a  valuable  figure  on  which  to  base 
comparisons. 

Two  practices  are  commonly  followed  in  expressing  results  of  pollination 
studies.  One  is  to  use  only  the  per  cent  fruit  obtained  from  the  original  number 
of  blossoms  pollinated,  the  other  (2)  is  to  express  results  as  "percentage  of  a 
commercial  crop."  It  is  pointed  out  by  those  who  follow  the  latter  practice 
that  to  express  results  only  in  terms  of  blossoms  that  set  fruit  is  to  ignore 
important  factors  which  influence  the  set  of  fruit,  particularly  in  view  of  the  fact 
that  a  percentage  of  fruit  that  might  be  ample  with  a  heavy  bloom,  would  be 
very  inadequate  when  the  bloom  was  light.  Furthermore,  the  same  percentage 
may  be  obtained  in  cases  in  which  widely  varying  distribution  of  the  fruit  on 
the  limbs  may  be  obtained.  Whatever  method  is  followed  is  arbitrary  and 
it  seems  to  be  just  as  difficult  to  lay  down  a  definite  rule  as  to  what  consitutes 
a  commercial  crop,  as  to  interpret  results  on  the  basis  of  percentage  fruit 
obtained,  which,  at  least,  forms  a  basis  of  comparison  between  varieties.  In 
our  work,  therefore,  we  have  chosen  to  use  the  figure  showing  the  percentage 
blossoms  that  develop  into  fruit.  Taking  into  account  the  normal  bearing 
habits  of  the  different  varieties  concerned  and  bearing  in  mind  the  fact  that  the 
practice  followed  was  to  select  for  pollination  only  limbs  in  good  bloom,  it  is 
considered  that  this  method  meets  the  needs  of  the  case  and  renders  intelligent 
comparisons  possible.  The  figure  of  five  per  cent,  selected  by  some  workers 
as  representing  a  commercial  set,  is  a  useful  one,  though,  as  indicated  elsewhere, 
the  bearing  habits  of  varieties  differ  and  the  proportion  of  blossoming  spurs 
would  have  an  important  influence. 

3.    POLLEN    TESTS 
(a)  METHOD 

In  order  to  keep  a  careful  check  upon  the  results  of  hand  pollinations,  all 
pollen  used  in  the  work  was  tested  for  germination.  Blossoms  were  gathered 
from  trees  of  the  required  varieties  before  fully  opened,  the  stamens  stripped 
of  their  anthers  by  means  of  forceps,  and  dried  in  the  sun  or  on  the  top  of 
an  electric  oven  running  at  50°C.  Ordinarily  the  pollen  was  stored  in  small 
open  petrie  dishes  in  desiccators  or  in  a  large  chamber  used  as  a  desiccator. 
Wherever  possible  mixed  samples  from  the  different  trees  were  secured,  to 
eliminate  error  due  to  differences  in  vigour  in  the  pollen  tree. 

Each  petrie  dish  was  labelled  with  the  name  of  the  variety  of  pollen,  and 
the  date  gathered.  Vials  for  use  in  the  orchard  were  correspondingly  labelled. 
As  the  pollinators  went  out  each  morning  the  pollen  was  transferred  from  the 
petrie  dish  to  the  vial  with  duplicate  label. 

The  daily  germination  tests  were  run  as  follows:  The  medium  15  per 
cent  cane  sugar  solution  with  -5  per  cent  agar  added,  was  sterilized  and  run 
into  a  number  of  test  tubes,  which  were  kept  plugged,  a  fresh  one  being  used 
for  each  day's  tests,  thus  eliminating  all  possibility  of  contamination  of  the 
medium. 


41 

Three  slides  were  put  up  with  each  variety  of  pollen,  the  pollen  being 
stirred  with  a  needle  into  the  agar  on  a  cover  slip,  which  was  then  inverted 
over  the  cavity  of  a  well  slide,  the  edge  of  the  well  being  coated  with  vaseline 
to  make  it  airtight.  After  24  hours  germination  at  room  temperature  the 
cover  slips  were  transferred  to  ordinary  slides  on  each  of  which  was  a  drop  of  5 
per  cent  lactic  acid.  The  acid  acted  as  a  killing  agent,  enabling  each  slide  to  be 
kept  for  several  days  and  read  at  leisure. 

Three  fields  containing  100-150  grains  were  read  on  each  slide  giving  a 
total  of  approximately  1,000  grains  for  each  variety.  At  the  beginning  of  the 
investigation  tests  were  run  on  various  germinative  media,  but  finally  15 
per  cent  sugar  agar  was  selected  as  being  most  satisfactory.  The  tests  were 
run  at  approximately  68°F. 

(b)  RESULTS 

The  accompanying  tables  indicate  the  results  by  years  and  show  the  actual 
per  cent  germination  obtained  from  the  pollen  samples  used  in  our  studies. 

TABLE  No.  7— GERMINATION  OF  APPLE  POLLEN  (1928-1932) 


Variety 

1928 

1929 

1930 

1931 

1932 

Annie  Elizabeth  (d) 

(3)53-75 
(3)  21-00 

(2)  45-27 

(2)  52-64 

(2)  21-75 

(3)  44-44 
(2)53-42 

(i)37-ll 

(3)  15-00 
(i)  60-00 
(2)  48-05 

(2)  10  00 
(2)  61-51 
(ii)38-40 

(2)  10-55 

(4)  33-94 

Baldwin  (t) 

(4)  0-22 

(3)56-60 

(2)85-18 

(2)  87-50 
(2)  52-20 

(2)47-61 

(«)  24-77 

(2)  92-50 

(3j  88-75 

(2)31-00 
(2)  46-75 
(i)  85-50 

(2)  46-75 

(2)80-12 
(2)  92-03 
(2)  93-00 
(2)  27-67 

(3)  10-67 

(4)  2-04 

(5)  3-67 
(i)  25-04 
(5)  2-31 
(3)  13-00 

(3)  53-95 

(4)  72-40 

Ben  Davis  (d) 

Bishop  Pippin  (Yellow  Bellflower)  (d) 

(4)  68-00 
/3)    5-60 

(2)  76-00 

(3)  38-90 

Blenheim  (t) 

700 

(<)  15-07 
(5)  2-66 
(8)  16-70 
(5)  20-00 

Chas.  Ross 

Cox  Orange  (d; 

29-50 

(3)  23-53 

(4)  54-25 
(<9  52-46 
(3)  56-29 

Crimson  Beauty  (t) 

Crimson  Bramley  (t) 

(3)  30-00 
(3)  27-24 
(3)  57-91 
(2)  80-83 

Delicious  (d) 

Duchess  (d) 

Emilie 

Fallawater 

Fameuse  (d) 

Golden  Russet  (d) 

1=)  25-51 

(3)  14-76 
(3)  76-25 

(7)  33-78 

(4)  46-10 

(6)  25-38 

(7)  35-69 

(5)  47.44 

(3)  44-29 

(4)  27-41 
(»)  24-00 
(4)  13-65 
(7)  17-21 
(i)  21-63 
(2)  18-56 

Gravenstein  (t) 

200 

Grimes  Golden  (d) 

Hubbardston 

Jonathan  (d) 

(3)  36-22 
(3)    1-66 

(3)  70-83 

(2)49-73 
(")  3-00 

(3)  70-62 

(3)  33-33 

(4)  74-40 
(3)58-27 
(3)51-00 

King  (t) 

15  00 

(5)  15-57 

Kinkead 

(s)  9-33 
(7)  1-20 
(3)  11-06 

Lane's  Prince  Albert 

Lipton 

Lobo 

(2)  70-67 

Longley  Pippin 

42 

TABLE  No.  7— GERMINATION  OF  APPLE  POLLEN  (1928-1932)— Cone. 


Variety 


1928     1929      1930      1931 


1932 


Melba 

Maiden's  Blush.  . 
Mam.  Blacktwig. 
Mcintosh 


(2)  38-67 


12  00 


(3)  14-67 

(4)  35-15 


Milwaukee 

Nonpareil  (Roxbury  Russet)  (t), 

Opalescent 

Ontario  (d) 

Ortley 

Red  Astrachan 

R.  R.  Beauty 

R.  W.  Reinette 

R.  I.  Greening  (t) 

Rome  Beauty 

Ribston  (t) 

Salome 

Stark  (t) 


(3)  15-44 


(»)  77-65 
(o  66-00 
(3)  32-11 
(2)  53-33 


(2)  40-20 

(2)  29-20 

(10)  36-25 


(2)89-86 


(U  15-76 
(3)  69-27 
(3)  0-59 
(-)  74-39 


Spy  (d). 


Seek-no-Further. . . 
Stayman  Winesap. 

St.  Lawrence 

Wagener  (d) 


(3)  24-00 
(3)  72-26 


41  00 


(»)  5211 
(3)  69-41 


(3)  50-00 
(i)48-36 
(«)  55-80 
(l0)  30-07 
(2)  0-00 
0)  40-53 

(2)  19-62 

(3)  64-82 
(3)  14-00 


0)  39-11 

(3)  68-40 
(2)  42-70 


Wellington  (d) 

Winter  Banana 

Wolf  River  (d) 

Worcester  Pearmain  (d) 

Yellow  Transparent 

York  Imperial 


(M  74-34 

57-00 

(3)  71-83 

(3)  100-0 

96-00 


(3)  90-00 
(>)  48-66 
(7)  33-53 


(2)  83-97 

(2)  48-00 

(*)  20-13 

(3)  70-00 
(2)  61-25 

(2)  36-66 

(3)  57-00 
(3)  25-22 

0)  86-89 

(2)  26-22 
(2)  62-20 


, ,   55 
(2)87-00 

-   .-,:■;, 

(2)89-ll 
i    92-tO 


(*)  58-03 
(0  57-05 

(2)  48-34 

(3)  38-59 


(3)  21-62 


(*)  19-23 
(»)  18-31 
(*)  23-32 
(3)  54-76 
(*)  68-35 


0)48-04 
(*)  73-28 
(3)  58-43 
{*)  60-56 
(*)  63-13 


Note  (!)—  In  1928-1929.  10'  „  sugar  was  the  medium  used. 

"  1930-32,  15%  sugar-agar  was  employed. 
Note  (2) — Figures  in  brackets  indicate  age  of  pollen  in  daws. 
Note  (»)— t=triploid;  (d)=diploid. 

Considerable  error  is  to  be  expected  in  germination  tests  in  spite  of  every 
possible  precaution  and  the  making  of  much  larger  counts  than  usually  con- 
sidered necessary.  Neither  should  it  be  supposed  that  germination  teste  made 
under  artificial  conditions  arc  exactly  indicative  of  the  fertilization  capacity  of 
pollen,  for  many  other  factors  are  involved.  It  will  be  noted,  however,  that 
some  varieties  show  a  uniformly  higher  germination  than  others.  Although  no 
direct  correlation  between  germination  and  set  has  been  arrived  at.  it  is  generally 
observed  that  a  pollen  with  high  germination  will  give  a  better  set  than  a  pollen 
with  low  germination;  sometimes  higher  than  would  be  expected,  sometimes 
lower.  Those  with  a  generally  low  percentage  germination  and  a  high  percentage 
of  aborted  and  shrivelled  grains  include  Blenheim.  Crimson  Bramley,  Baldwin. 
R.  I.  Greening,  Gravenstein,  Bramley,  King  and  Ribston  which  are  triploids, 
to  which  might  be  added  Nonpareil  and  Stark,  and  several  others  which,  judging 
from  their  behaviour  in  this  and  other  respects  will  probably  be  found  to  be 
triploids;  while  of  the  known  diploids.  Spy.  Delicious,  Ben  Davis.  Jonathan. 
Yellow  Transparent,  Ontario,  Red  Astrachan  and  others  have  a  relatively  higher 
germination  and  a  lower  proportion  of  aborted  grains. 

In  this  connection  Einset  (1930)  states:  "  In  view  of  the  eytologiea! 
evidence,  it  seems  apparent  that  the  low  pollen  germination  is  not  directly 
responsible   for   the   ineffectiveness   of    the   triploid    varieties.    Gravenstein    and 


43 

Baldwin,  as  pollinizers.  Even  though  the  percentage  of  viable  pollen  is  low, 
there  should  remain  a  sufficient  supply  in  many  cases  to  insure  fertilization  and 
subsequent  seed  development.  It  seems  obvious,  however,  that  the  irregular 
chromosome   distribution,  which   has   supposedly   cut   down   the   percentage   of 


# 


* 


Af 


w* 


4 

• 

0        i  '      ■» « 

ft 

# 

«r 

p  * 

• 

**    * 

«* 

%  ."* 

jj      o 

»i-j®t^ 

4 

5    K  ■                      6 

Fig.  7. — Germinating  pollen  from  certain  apple  varieties: 
(1)    pollen    of    Baldwin;     (2)    King;     (3)    Wagener;     (4) 
Golden  Russet;    (5)   Spy;    (6)   Gravenstein   (original). 

viable  pollen,  is  now  causing  further  abortion  in  the  zygotes."  This  agrees  with 
the  observations  of  Kobel  (1930  and  1931)  that  a  high  pollen  germination  is 
characteristic  of  diploid  varieties  -and  a  low  germination  of  triploid  varieties. 
The  results  of  all  our  tests  are  in  agreement  with  the  foregoing  conclusions. 


44 


An  interesting  feature  brought  out  by  our  tables  is  the  variability  of  the 
results  of  germination  tests.  There  is  undoubtedly  a  large  factor  of  error  in 
this  work,  but  it  is  apparent  that  in  diploid  varieties  the  pollen  is  generally 
good,  in  triploids  generally  poor.  Furthermore,  in  some  seasons  the  germination 
is  proportionally  better  than  in  other  seasons.  As  already  indicated  the  pollen 
tests  carried  out  in  connection  with  the  foregoing  studies  were  undertaken  solely 
as  a  check  on  the  germination  of  the  samples  used  and,  as  such,  appear  to  be 
reasonably  adequate.  Heilborn  (1932)  rightly  points  out  that  most  germina- 
tion percentages  are  based  on  too  few  counts.  He  further  contends  that  a 
proper  comparison  between  the  pollen  of  different  varieties  cannot  be  based  upon 
percentage  figures  of  germination,  determined  without  due  regard  to  the  amount 
of  empty  pollen  grains  characteristic  of  each  variety  and  without  a  very  strict 
control  of  the  conditions  prevailing  during  germination  experiments.  He  con- 
siders that  percentage  germination  figures  bring  out  one  thing  only,  viz.,  the 
profound  difference  between  diploid  and  triploid  sorts.  He  contends  that  all 
differences  in  pollen  morphology  are  not  due  to  chromosome  aberation  and  that 
triploid  and  diploid  varieties  must  be  treated  separately.  In  evaluating  diploid 
varieties  he  considers  it  essential  to  use  viability  figures  i.e.  germinability  instead 
of  germination  percentage.  He  divides  diploid  varieties  into  three  categories  on 
this  basis  and  concludes  that  in  such  varieties  there  is  a  sharp  distinction 
between  wholly  fertile  and  partially  sterile  varieties,  of  which  only  the  first 
mentioned  should  be  regarded  as  perfect  pollen  producers.  He  presents  evidence 
to  show  that  the  sterility  has  a  genetical  basis  and  is,  to  a  high  degree  at  least. 
independent  of  climatic  or  metabolic  conditions,  being  caused  by  lethal  gene 
combinations1. 

(i)  Other  Data. — Experiments  to  determine  the  optimum  temperature  for 
pollen  germination  under  artificial  conditions,  showed  considerable  apparent 
difference  between  varieties,  the  point  varying  between  17.8°  C.  and  25.6°  C, 
Germination  occurred  from  all  temperatures  above  freezing  to  about  57°  C,  but 


IO  15  SO  25  SO 

sd?<?  o/  /h//e/?  //?  Dat/s       I  3&/aW//7 

Fig.  8.- -Influence   of    age   on   germinability   of    Baldwin   pollen 
under  different  conditions  of  storage    (original). 

at  the  lower  temperatures  pollen-tube  growth  was  very  slow,  finally,  however, 
attaining  the  length  characteristic  of  the  variety.  Pollen  retains  its  viability 
best  when  stored  in  open  petrie  dishes  in  desiccators,  especially  in  damp  weather. 
Under  these  conditions  good  germination  is  obtained  for  several  days.  Northern 
Spy  showed  greatest  longevity  under  the  conditions  of  the  test,  germinating  up 
to  29  per  cent  when  10  days  old,  3.06  per  cent  at  23  days,  but  no  germination 
at  30  days.    Germination  at  3  days  was  70  per  cent.    Where  stigmas  of  apple 


45 


Open  tnroom 
Open  /n  cfess/cafor 


//7  via/  incfessica/or" 


o  *-■ 


5  "10  15  20  25 

s4/?e  o/Po//es?  /n  Days  E  Grare/7s/e//? 


so 


Fig.  9. — Influence   of   age  on   germinability   of   Gravenstein   pollen    under    different 
conditions  of  storage    (original). 


Open /r?  room 
Oper?  m  afess/cafor 
//?  ria/  //?  cfess/ca/or 


Fig.  10. 


^?<?e  ofPo//e/7  //?  £&*/&      M  /C/>?? 

-Influence  of  age  on  germinability  of  King  pollen  under  different  conditions  of  storage 

(original) . 


46 


varieties  were  introduced  into  the  germinative  media  there  was  no  evidence  of 
any  effect  on  germination.  Where  foreign  stigmas  were  introduced  some  of  them 
produced  a  lethal  effect. 

In  experiments  in  carriage  of  pollen  by  wind,  glass  slides  were  smeared  with 
liquid  petrolatum  and  placed  in  spore  traps  of  the  weather  vane  type  at  the  edge 
of  the  orchards  at  varying  distances  from  the  trees.  Apple  pollen  was  found  on 
all  the  slides  up  to  200  ft.,  the  counts  running  as  follows:  25  ft.,  325  grains;  50 
ft.,  267  grains;  75  ft.,  169  grains;  100  ft.,  80  grains;  150  ft.,  46  grains;  and  200^ 


ai 

80 

, 

70 

J 

V V    1 

1 
|60 

V\  ,1 

\ 
\ 
\ 

KP-^ 

^ 

V 

\ 
\ 

Open  //?  roo/r 

? 

k 

\ 

\ 
{    \ 

\         \      y\ 

Operr  //7  tfess 

. 

§50 

vc&ror 

\         X/   \ 

//7ria///7  cfess/crerror 

t 

\                \ 

t\ 

V      \ 

1 

\       \ 

^40 

\   \ 

<5 

^"u 

I 

\ 

ft 

u 

>30 

// 

1   1 

\ 

$ 

I 

J\i    1 

\ 

fc 

\ 
\ 

/  \\  1 

\ 

* 

f    w 

\ 

\ 

\ 

?fl 

1    \ 

in 

\ 
\ 
•'  :        \ 
\ 

\ 

^■>» 

^s 

.> 

5  IO  15  20  25 


30 


Fig.  11. — Influence  of  age  on  germinabilits    of  Spy  pollen  under  different   conditions  of  storage 

(original  I . 


47 

ft.,  38  grains.  This  confirms  the  observations  of  Hockey  and  Harrison  (1930) 
carried  out  under  identical  conditions.  Not  all  the  pollen,  however,  was  wind 
carried.  In  fact,  more  pollen  was  placed  on  the  slide  by  insects  as  indicated  by 
its  occurrence  in  clumps  of  35  to  50  grains,  while  hairs  and  pieces  of  insects  legs 
and  wings  were  found  on  the  slides.  In  other  cases  the  scattered  nature  of  the 
pollen  did  seem  to  indicate  carriage  by  wind.  In  1932  also,  an  experiment  was 
conducted  in  which  a  current  of  air  from  an  orchard  duster  was  blown  thoroughly 
through  a  large  bouquet  of  blossoming  apple  limbs  adjacent  to  a  tree  also  in 
bloom.  The  large  set  obtained  on  this  side  of  the  tree,  equivalent  to  that  obtained 
on  hand  pollinated  limbs,  indicated  that  the  pollen  had  been  well  distributed  by 
this  method.  In  spite  of  the  foregoing,  it  appears  that  under  normal  conditions 
the  effect  of  wind  carriage  of  pollen  is  very  local  and  is  not  an  important  factor 
in  the  pollination  of  the  apple. 

Experiments  in  the  rate  of  pollen  tube  growth  were  made  by  pollinating  Spy 
blossoms  with  pollen  of  different  varieties  and  clipping  the  stigmas  at  definite 
intervals  thereafter,  and  noting  the  set.  Considerable  differences  were  noted  in 
the  time  required  to  effect  fertilization,  as  measured  by  the  set  secured,  between 
the  results  obtained  in  1928  and  those  obtained  in  1931,  this  difference  being 
apparently  correlated  with  weather  conditions.  In  1931  maximum  sets  were 
obtained  in  48  hours,  whereas  in  1928,  the  60-hour  interval  gave  optimum  results. 
Little  consistent  difference  was  noted  between  the  different  pollen  varieties,  except 
when  Spy  itself  was  used,  in  which  case  fertilization  was  delayed,  but  these 
selling  tests  only  gave  a  set  of  -37  per  cent,  which  is  too  small  a  number  upon 
which  to  base  conclusions. 


4.  EXPERIMENTAL    RESULTS    OF    POLLINATION    EXPERIMENTS 
WITH    STANDARD    VARIETIES 

The  results  of  all  experiments  with  standard  varieties  are  summarized  in 
the  following  tables  and  discussion.  In  view  of  the  importance  of  chromosome 
constitution  in  the  behaviour  of  varieties,  it  has  been  considered  advisable  to 
classify  the  crosses  as,  (1)  diploid  x  diploid,  (2)  triploid  x  diploid,  (3)  diploid  x 
triploid  and  (4)  triploid  x  triploid. 

Each  variety  is  then  discussed  in  detail  from  the  following  standpoints: — 
(i)   Results  obtained  by  other  workers. 

(ii)  Value  from  the  standpoint  of  self-fruitfulness  as  determined  by  "set  " 
and  "  fruit  "  secured. 

(iii)  Value  as  a  female  parent,  using  the  same  criteria. 

(iv)  Value  as  a  male  parent  considering  "  set,"  "  fruit "  and  "  seeds  "  pro- 
duced. The  "  per  cent  seeds  "  in  our  tables  is  calculated  from  the  1930  and  1931 
figures  only,  as  indicated  below: — 


Variety 

Total 
blossoms 

Total 
seeds 

Baldwin 

18,985 
22,388 
21,584 
16,613 
22,742 
4,767 

1,410 

Cox  Orange 

11,548 

Golden  Russet 

11,972 

Gravenstein 

3,117 

King 7. 

2,820 

Spy 

2,578 

(v)   Evidence  from  tent  series, 
(vi)   Summary  of  results  with  all  varieties, 
(vii)  Other  data,  if  any. 
(viii)   General  summary  for  variety. 

Finally,  a  general  summary  based  on  the  results  of  tent  and  hand  pollination 
studies,  with  special  emphasis  upon  findings  of  a  fundamental  nature,  is  presented. 


48 


(a)  BALDWIN 

(i)  Results  of  Other  Workers. — MacDaniels  (1927)  in  a  summary  of  pollina- 
tion studies  with  this  variety,  illustrates  that  self-fruitfulness  is  not  a  fixed  factor 
in  that  or  any  variety,  but  varied  greatly  according  to  differences  in  environ- 
mental or  other  conditions.  Other  workers,  Overholser,  (1927),  Morris  (1921), 
MacDaniels  and  Heinicke  (1929)  and  Howlett  (1927)  all  record  this  variety  as 
either  self-fruitful  or  partially  self-fruitful.  Furthermore,  all  workers  record 
Baldwin  as  an  excellent  female  parent,  but  poor,  or  at  best,  only  fair  as  a  male 
parent. 

(ii)  Results  of  Selfing  Tests  on  Baldwin  (1928-1932). 


Total  blossoms 

Total  set 

Per  cent  set 

Total  fruit 

Per  cent  fruit 

6,856 

1,711 

24-96 

437 

6-37 

The  results  of  hand  pollination  tests  at  the  Experimental  Station  with  this 
variety,  indicate  that  it  is  very  self-fruitful  compared  with  all  other  varieties 
tested.  The  selfing  tests  over  a  period  of  five  years  show  a  percentage  "  set "  of 
24-96  and  of  "fruit"  6-37.  It  should  be  emphasized,  however,  that,  though 
Baldwin  is  very  self-fruitful  and  gives,  when  selfed,  a  better  yield  than  with 
many  other  varieties,  it  does  not  give  as  good  results  as  when  crossed  with  highly 
cross-fruitful  varieties  such  as  Cox  Orange. 


(iii)  Results  from  Baldwin 

as  Female  Parent  (1928-1932). 

Total  blossoms 

Total  set 

Per  cent  set 

Total  fruit 

Per  cent  fruit 

28,178 

11,672 

41-42 

3,232 

11-47 

Its  value  as  a  female  parent  is  high,  as  indicated  in  the  above  table,  giving 
a  percentage  "set"  of  41-42  and  a  percentage  "fruit"  of  11-47.  All  diploids 
tested  on  Baldwin,  such  as  Cox  Orange,  Golden  Russet,  and  Spy,  proved  to  be 
effective  pollinizers  for  the  variety.  Mcintosh  and  Wagener  were  tested  in  1932 
only,  and  gave  every  indication  of  being  equally  as  effective  as  the  other  diploids 
tested  over  the  longer  period. 


(iv)  Results  from  Baldwin  as  Male  Parent  (1928-1932). 

Total 
blossoms 

Total 
set 

Per  cent 
set 

Total 
fruit 

Per  cent 
fruit 

Per  cent 
seeds 

26,841 

2,974 

11-08 

681                        2-54 

7-43 

By  all  criteria  used  Baldwin  gives  relatively  poor  results  as  a  male  parent, 
the  per  cent  "set"  for  the  five-year  period  being  11-08,  the  per  cent  "fruit" 
2-54,  and  the  per  cent  seed  7-43. 

(v)  Evidence  from  Tented  Series. — Additional  evidence  from  tent  studies 
shows  that  self-fruitfulness  takes  place  to  a  very  marked  degree,  the  tented  trees 
(with  bees  and  no  bouquets)  over  a  four-year  period  (1929-1932)  giving  an 
average  percentage  fruit  of  7-77.  The  untouched  tree  (no  bees  and  no  bouquets), 
over  the  same  period,  only  gave  a  percentage  fruit  of  3-49,  indicating  that  selfing 
cannot  take  place  to  a  satisfactory  degree  without  the  aid  of  pollinating  insects. 
The  open  pollinated  tree,  representing  results  from  a  mixture  of  various  pollens, 


Fig.  12. — Photographs  illustrating  results  of  experimental  pol- 
linations: (1)  Baldwin  selfed;  (2)  Baldwin  x  Cox  Orange; 
(3)    Baldwin  x  King   (original). 


60796—4 


50 


gave  10-40  per  cent  fruit  over  a  four-year  average,  while  the  tree  with  bees  and 
Blenheim  bouquets,  over  the  same  period,  resulted  in  4-96  per  cent,  indicating 
cross-unfruitfulness  between  these  two  varieties.  On  hand  pollinated  limbs  used 
in  the  tent  studies,  we  find  an  effective  pollinizer  like  Cox  Orange  to  give  an 
extremely  high  percentage,  where  the  yield  over  the  whole  tree  is  reduced  as  in 
the  case  of  the  treatments,  e.g.,  the  trees  with  no  bees  and  no  bouquets.  Limbs 
hand  pollinated  with  an  ineffective  pollinizer  in  the  tented  series  gave  a  low  per- 
centage fruit,  indicating  similar  results  to  that  of  whole  trees  tented  and  treated 
in  a  similar  manner. 

(vi)  Summary  of  Results  with  all  Varieties  on  Baldwin  (1928-1932). 


Cox  Orange 

G.  Russet 

Graven  stein 

King 

Spy 

Total 
blossoms 

Per  cent 
fruit 

Total 
blossoms 

Per  cent 
fruit 

Total 
blossoms 

Per  cent 
fruit 

Total 
blossoms 

Per  cent 
fruit 

Total 
blossoms 

Per  cent 

fruit 

5,718 

14-64 

5,761 

12-25 

5,186 

8-93 

5,436 

8-65 

6,077 

12-44 

The  above  tests  show  that  the  triploicl  varieties,  mainly  Gravenstein  and 
King,  gave  satisfactory  results  when  used  as  male  parents  on  Baldwin.  However, 
the  possibility  that  a  percentage  of  this  is  due  to  selfing  cannot  be  excluded,  and, 
if  emasculation  tests  were  practised,  the  results  from  these  sorts  might  have  been 
lower.  That  selfing  may  not  be  the  entire  explanation  is  indicated  by  the  fact 
that  the  pollen  of  certain  triploids  when  used  on  Baldwin  gave  a  percentage  fruit 
considerably  in  excess  of  those  obtained  by  selfing,  as  indicated  elsewhere. 

This  brings  forth  the  necessity  of  further  work  under  emasculated  condi- 
tions to  determine  the  true  value  of  these  male  parents.  All  the  diploids  tested, 
Cox  Orange,  Golden  Russet  and  Spy  gave  slightly  higher  results  than  the  men- 
tioned triploids  and  may  be  readily  termed  as  excellent  pollinizers  for  Baldwin. 

(vii)  Other  Data.- A  considerable  number  of  blossoms  were  pollinated  with 
R.  I.  Greening  pollen  during  the  season  of  1932  to  determine  its  value  as  a  male 
parent.  The  results  from  this  triploid  were  very  unusual,  a  percentage  fruit  of 
14-86  being  obtained  from  a  population  of  1,521  blossoms.  Other  triploids  such 
as  Nonpareil  and  Blenheim  gave  depressing  effects  when  used  as  bouquets  in 
tented  trees  with  bees. 

(viii)  General  Summary  for  Variety. — This  variety  exhibits  a  high  degree 
of  self-fruitfulness,  24-96  per  cent  "  set"  and  6-37  per  cent  "  fruit"  over  five- 
year  period,  the  highest  for  any  variety  tested.  The  bagged  and  untouched  blos- 
soms gave  a  very  low  per  cent  fruit,  ranging  from  3-70  on  the  open  pollinated 
trees,  to  1-01  on  the  tented  trees  supplied  with  bees  and  no  bouquets,  i.e.  selfed, 
over  a  four-year  period.  As  a  female  parent  Baldwin  stands  high,  giving  an 
average  of  11-47  per  cent  for  the  varieties  tested  over  a  period  of  five  years,  this 
figure  being  larger  than  the  open  pollinated  average  of  10-40  per  cent  for  a  four- 
year  period.  Baldwin,  being  classified  as  a  triploid,  might  be  expected  to.  and 
actually  does,  give  relatively  poor  results  as  a  male  parent,  2-54  per  cent  being 
obtained  on  the  varieties  tested. 

It  should  be  understood  that  Baldwin  is  a  markedly  biennial  bearer  and 
that  our  tests  had,  therefore,  to  be  made  on  different  tree-  each  year,  trees  being- 
selected  that  had  not  blossomed  or  had  not  blossomed  heavily  the  previous  year. 
This  habit  markedly  diminishes  its  commercial  value.  It  is  known,  however,  as 
a  reliable  cropper  every  other  year,  almost  invariably  bearing  crops  when  bloom 
is  obtained,  whereas,  certain  other  varieties  are  notably  uncertain.  The  high 
degree  of  self-fruitfulness  exhibited  by  this  variety  is.  no  doubt,  associated  with 
this  habit. 


51 


(b)  COX   ORANGE 

(i)  Results  of  Other  Workers. — Crane  (1926)  lists  this  variety  as  partially 
self-fruitful,  and  points  out  that  the  trees  bear  early.  Only  moderate  crops 
result  from  selfing  and  full  crops  when  crossed.  Auchter  (1921)  of  Maryland 
classifies  Cox  Orange  as  self-fruitful,  while  on  the  other  hand,  Corrie  (1916), 
Sutton  (1919)  and  Hooper  (1921)  place  it  as  self-sterile  or  self-unfruitful.  Crane 
and  Lawrence  (1930)  classify  it  as  a  diploid,  thus  indicating  its  value  as  an 
effective  male  parent. 

(ii)   Results  of  Selfing  Tests  on  Cox  Orange  (1928-1932). 


Total  blossoms 

Total  set 

Per  cent  set 

Total  fruit 

Per  cent  fruit 

9,016 

1,136 

12-60 

163 

1-81 

The  hand  pollinated  results  at  Kentville  indicate  that  this  variety  gave  a 
low  percentage  "  set "  and  "  fruit "  in  selfing  tests,  the  average  for  five  years 
being  12-60  and  1-81  respectively.  The  latter  figure  indicates  a  low  value  from 
the  standpoint  of  self-fruitfulness  and  the  necessity  of  the  provision  of  cross- 
fruitful  varieties  in  order  to  secure  commercial  crops. 

(iii)  Results  from  Cox  Orange  as  Female  Parent  (1928-1932). 


Total  blossoms 

Total  set 

Per  cent  set 

Total  fruit 

Per  cent  fruit 

37,091 

10,526 

28-38 

2,869 

7-74 

As  a  female  parent  the  average  figure  over  a  five-year  period  was  7-74, 
this  result  being  obtained  by  using  pollen  from  standard  varieties.  This  figure  is 
higher  than  that  obtained  from  open  pollinated  trees,  which  gave  a  percentage 
fruit  of  6-74,  over  a  four-year  period.  All  diploids  tested,  Golden  Russet, 
Mcintosh  and  Wagener,  proved  effective  pollinizers  on  Cox,  excellent  results  being 
evident  in  each  case. 


(iv)   Result 

s  from  Cox 

Orange  as  Male  Parent  (1928-1932) 

Total 
blossoms 

Total 
set 

Per  cent 
set 

Total 
fruit 

Per  cent 
fruit 

Per  cent 
seeds 

27,890 

9,638 

34-56 

3,202 

11-48 

51-58 

As  a  male  parent  this  variety  proved  excellent  in  all  cases,  the  five-year 
average  being  11-48  per  cent  fruit.  This  result  is  in  line  with  that  obtained  with 
all  diploid  varieties  tested  and  enhances  the  value  of  this  variety  in  commercial 
plantings.  The  high  percentage  seed  obtained  by  the  use  of  this  variety  is  con- 
firmatory of  the  "  set  "  and  "  fruit  "  figures. 

(v)  Evidence  from  Tented  Series. — This  variety  was  not  included  in  the 
tented  series,  thus  our  information,  pertaining  to  self-fruitfulness,  is  confined 
entirely  to  hand  pollination  tests. 


60796—41 


52 


dhk  \*^.±2*~ 


**YTr.i7? 


Ftg.  13. — Photographs  illustrating  results  of  experimental  pollina- 
tions: (1)  Cox  Orange  selfed;  (2)  Cox  Orange  x  Golden  Russet; 
(3)  Cox  Orange  x  King   (original). 


53 

(vi)  Summary  of  Results  with  All  Varieties  on  Cox  Orange  (1928-1932). 


Baldwin 

Graven  stein 

Golden  Russet 

Total 
blossoms 

Per  cent 
fruit 

Total 
blossoms 

Per  cent 
fruit 

Total 
blossoms 

Per  cent 
fruit 

6,376 

2-51 

6,586 

4-97 

6,190 

13-99 

King 

Mcintosh 

Wagener 

6,887 

2-47 

5,186 

10-99 

5,866 

13-23 

The  triploid  varieties,  namely,  Baldwin,  Gravenstein,  and  King,  gave  uni- 
formly poor  results  on  Cox.  The  diploids,  Golden  Russet,  Mcintosh  and  Wagener. 
on  the  other  hand,  gave  excellent  results  throughout,  indicating  clearly  that  onl> 
such  sorts  should  be  used  to  pollinate  Cox  Orange. 

(vii)  Other  Data. — To  illustrate  the  value  of  cross-pollination,  where  solid 
blocks  of  one  variety  are  planted,  several  limbs  were  hand  pollinated  with  Cox 
Orange  pollen  in  a  ten-acre  block  of  solid  Blenheim  at  Lakeville.  The  results 
were  very  significant,  since,  in  all  cases  where  Cox  was  used,  a  heavy  percentage 
fruit,  varying  from  20  to  75,  resulted,  this  being  an  extreme  contrast  to  the 
remainder  of  the  block  in  which  the  set  was  light  and  scattering,  in  most  cases 
below  one  per  cent.  This  experiment  was  repeated  on  a  small  scale  in  a  large 
number  of  orchards  where  Blenheim  were  reported  to  be  giving  poor  returns. 
In  all  cases,  the  results  were  marked  and,  in  many,  very  spectacular;  the  hand 
pollinated  limb  being  laden  with  fruit  in  contrast  to  the  other  limbs  with  few 
or  none.    Similar,  but  less  spectacular  results  were  obtained  on  Stark. 

(viii)  General  Summary  for  Variety. — This  variety  may  be  considered 
to  be  commercially  self-unfruitful  on  the  basis  of  a  five-year  average,  although  a 
low  percentage  of  fruit  was  obtained  in  most  selfing  tests.  It  is  an  excellent  male 
parent,  giving  satisfactory  results  for  all  varieties  that  blossom  at  approximately 
the  same  time  and,  being  somewhat  irregular  in  its  blossoming  habits,  it  is  useful 
on  an  unusually  large  range  of  varieties.  As  female  parent  in  hand  pollinated 
tests,  Cox  has  given  a  high  average  percentage  fruit,  the  figure  averaging  higher 
than  the  open  pollinated  results. 

(c)  GOLDEN  RUSSET 

(i)  Results  of  Other  Workers. — Sax  (1922)  states,  that,  for  practical  pur- 
poses, all  Maine  apples  are  "  self  sterile "  from  the  commercial  standpoint. 
Ben  Davis,  Baldwin,  Golden  Russet,  R.  I.  Greening,  Northern  Spy  are  all  men- 
tioned as  "  interfertile,"  with  the  exception  of  Greening  and  Baldwin,  as  pollen 
parents. 


(ii)  Results 

of  Selfing  Tests 

on  Golden  Russet  (1928-1932) 

Total  blossoms 

Total  set 

Per  sent  set 

Total  fruit 

Per  cent  fruit 

8,016 

1,009 

12-59 

139 

1-73 

Golden  Russet  shows  a  fair  degree  of  self-fruitfulness  under  the  conditions 
of  these  experiments.  This  variety  is  rather  variable  in  its  bearing  habits,  and 
when  grown  on  the  heavier  soils  appears  to  be  more  fruitful.  The  average  figure 
from  selfing  over  five  years  is  12-59  per  cent  "  set "  and  1-73  "  fruit,"  indicating  ? 
low  average  value  from  the  standpoint  of  self-fruitfulness. 


54 


.*  V 


Fig  14.— Photographs  illustrating  results  of  experimental  pollinations:  (1)  Golden 
Russet  self ed;  (2)  Golden  Russet  x  Mcintosh;  (3-10)  Golden  Russet  x  Baldwin; 
(3-11)  Golden  Russet  selfed   (original). 


55 

(iii)   Results  from  Golden  Russet  as  Female  Parent  (1928-1932) 


Total  blossoms 

Total  set 

Per  cent  set 

Total  fruit 

Per  cent  fruit 

25,377 

5,335 

21-02 

1,042 

411 

Our  results  show  Russet  to  be  rather  poor  as  a  female  parent,  the  average 
being  21-02  per  cent  "  set  "  and  4-11  "fruit"  (1928-1932).  Mcintosh  and  Cox 
Orange  were  the  only  diploids  tested  on  this  variety,  both  proving  very  satis- 
factory pollinizers.  As  indicated  previously,  the  percentage  fruit  to  bloom  is 
considerably  lower  on  the  light  sandy  soil  at  Kentville  than  appears  to  be  the  case 
on  the  heavier  types  of  soil,  such  as  are  found  in  Lakeville  and  similar  districts. 
Thus,  the  value  of  Golden  Russet  as  a  female  parent  may  vary  to  a  marked 
degree,  depending  upon  the  locality  in  which  the  tests  are  made  and  the  con- 
ditions under  which  it  is  grown. 

(iv)  Results  from  Golden  Russet  as  Male  Parent  (1928-1932). 


Total 
blossoms 

Total 

set 

Per  cent 

set 

Total 
fruit 

Per  cent 
fruit 

Per  cent 
seeds 

28,341 

10,993 

38-79 

3,185 

11-24 

55-47 

As  a  male  parent  Golden  Russet  is  in  the  front  rank  giving  an  average  per 
cent  "set"  and  "fruit"  and  "seed"  of  38-79,  11-24  and  55-47  respectively. 
Being  a  desirable  commercial  variety  it  is  particularly  valuable  as  a  poilinizer  in 
any  scheme  of  orchard  planting. 

(v)  Evidence  from  Tent  Series. — Tented  tests  were  not  conducted  with 
this  variety,  thus  all  information  available  as  to  its  self-fruitfulness  is  limited  to 
the  hand  pollinated  tests. 


(vi)  Summary  of  Results  with  All  Varieties  on  Golden  Russet  (1928-1932). 


Baldwin 

Cox  Orange 

Gravenstein 

King 

Mcintosh 

Total 
blossoms 

Per  cent 
fruit 

Total 
blossoms 

Per  cent 
fruit 

Total 
blossoms 

Per  cent 
fruit 

Total 
blossoms 

Per  cent 

fruit 

Total 
blossoms 

Per  cent 
fruit 

5,333 

2-33 

5,355 

7-51 

4,995           2-28 

5,767 

2-15 

3,927 

7-08 

Baldwin,  Gravenstein  and  King  varieties  gave  poor  results  throughout,  as  one 
might  well  expect,  these  varieties  being  triploids.  Cox  Orange  and  Mcintosh,  as 
pointed  out  previously,  produced  excellent  results  and  one  may  reasonably 
assume,  on  the  basis  of  similar  tests,  that  any  diploid,  with  suitable  overlapping 
of  the  blossoming  periods,  would  prove  satisfactory  as  a  poilinizer  for  Golden 
Russet. 

(vii)  Other  Data. — To  illustrate  further  the  point  regarding  the  varying 
degrees  of  self-fruitfulness  in  relation  to  locality,  we  find  that  the  hand  pollinated 
tests  conducted  in  a  Golden  Russet  orchard  on  a  heavy  loam  soil  at  Wolfville 
gave  a  percentage  fruit  of  6-34  as  a  female  parent.  This  figure  is  over  two  per 
cent  higher  than  the  five-year  average.  This  is  in  line  with  the  current  opinion 
with  respect  to  this  variety,  though  the  possibility  of  other  factors  influencing 
the  foregoing  result  is  not  excluded. 


56 


(viii)  General  Summary  for  Variety. — Although  Golden  Russet  is  listed 
as  "  self  sterile  "  in  Maine,  our  selfing  results  indicate  a  rather  low  degree  of 
self-fruitfulness,  with  considerable  variation  from  year  to  year.  As  a  male 
parent,  it  is  very  effective,  being  equally  as  good  as  Cox  Orange  and  Spy  in  this 
respect.  Its  value  as  a  female  parent  has  been  elaborated  in  the  foregoing  dis- 
cussion and  may  be  considered  of  variable  nature,  tending  to  be  poor  on  the 
lighter  types  of  soil.  Mcintosh  and  Cox  Orange  are  the  only  satisfactory  pol- 
linizers  tested. 

(d)  GRAVENSTEIN 

(i)  Results  of  Other  Workers.  Overholser  (1927)  states  that  this  variety 
is  self-unfruitful,  giving  a  per  cent  set  of  -09.  His  work  further  showed  that 
Gravenstein  was  an  ineffective  pollinizer  for  R.  I.  Greening,  Jonathan,  Delicious 
and  Baldwin.  He  found  Delicious  to  be  an  effective  pollinizer  for  the  variety. 
Morris  (1921)  lists  Gravenstein  as  a  partially  self-fruitful  variety,  as  also  does 
Vincent  (1915).  However,  we  find  the  latter  cases  to  be  the  only  exceptions  to 
the  general  belief  that  Gravenstein  is  highly  self-unfruitful.  Wellington,  Stout, 
et  at.  (1929),  working  in  New  York  state,  obtained  no  set  where  selfing  was 
practised.  The  majority  of  writers  record  Gravenstein  as  a  good  female  parent 
and  wherever  diploid  varieties  are  used  as  male  parents,  good  yields  are  obtained. 
On  the  other  hand,  due  to  the  fact  that  it  is  a  triploid,  we  find  it  giving  poor 
results  wherever  used  as  a  male  parent. 


(ii)   Results 

of  Selfing  Tests  on  Gravenstein  (1928-1932). 

Total  blossoms 

T  otal  set 

Per  cent  set 

Total  fruit 

Per  cent  fruit 

7944 

193 

2-43 

91 

115 

In  the  hand  pollinated  selfing  tests  at  Kentville.  Gravenstein  has  proven 
to  be  the  most  self-unfruitful  of  the  six  standard  varieties  under  test,  the  average 
for  five  years  being  2-43  per  cent  "  set"  and  1-15  per  cent  "  fruit,"  the  figures 
based  on  a  count  of  nearly  8,000  blossoms. 

(iii)   Results  from  Gravenstein  as  Female  Parent  (1928-1932). 


Total  blossoms 

Total  set 

Per  cent  se1 

Total  fruit 

Per  cent  fruit 

32,225 

6,876 

21-31 

3,048 

9-46 

As  a  female  parent,  Gravenstein  may  be  considered  good,  giving  average 
per  cent  "set"  of  21-34  and  "fruit"  9:46.  Cox  Orange,  Mcintosh,  Golden 
Russet  and  Wagener,  all  diploids,  are  excellent  pollinizers  for  the  variety  and 
when  interplanted  should  give  maximum  results. 


(iv)   Results  from  Gravenstein  as  Male  Parent  (1928-1932). 

Total  blossoms 

Total  set 

Per  cent  set 

Total  fruit 

Per  cent  fruit 

Per  rein  seeds 

21,911 

4,729 

21-58 

1.114 

5-08 

18-76 

As  a  male  parent,  although  not  as  poor  as  Baldwin  and  King,  it  has  not 
been  at  all  satisfactory.  The  results  show  an  average  percentage  of  21-58 
"set,"  5-08  "fruit"  and  18-76  seeds.  The  latter  figures  are  higher" than  might 
be  expected,  keeping  in  mind  the  fact  that  this  variety  is  a  triploid  form.  Of  all 
this  group  it  has  given  in  our  tests  the  highest  value  as  a  male  parent. 


57 


FlG.  15.— Photographs  illustrating  results  of  experimental  pol- 
linations: (1)  Gravenstein  selfed;  (2)  Gravenstein  x  Wag- 
ener;    (3)   Gravenstein  x  King  (original). 


5S 


(v)  Evidence  from  Tented  Series. — It  is  interesting  to  note  the  degree  of 
self- fruit f illness  which  occurs  in  the  tented  series  over  a  period  of  four  years. 
The  tented  tree  with  bees  and  no  bouquets  (selfed)  gave  an  average  of  2-12  per 
cent  fruit.  This  figure  would  tend  to  make  one  believe  that  Gravenstein  is  more 
self-fruitful  than  hand  pollination  tests  would  appear  to  indicate.  The  cutting 
back  of  the  trees  in  order  to  put  the  tents  in  place  appeared  to  increase  the 
number  of  seedless  fruit,  which  Gravenstein  has  a  tendency  to  produce.  On  the 
other  hand  the  trees  with  no  bees  and  no  bouquets  gave  an  average  of  0-67  per 
cent  fruit.  This  indicates  clearly  that  natural  selfing  in  the  absence  of  bees  is 
exhibited  only  to  a  very  limited  degree.  Open  pollinated  trees  showed  the  high 
percentage  fruit  of  9-91  and,  where  an  effective  pollinizer,  viz.,  Wagener,  was 
used,  10-90  per  cent  of  fruit  was  obtained.  In  the  cases  where  B-lenheim 
bouquets  were  introduced  we  find  the  average  over  the  four  years  reduced  to 
1-14  per  cent  fruit,  the  latter  figure  being  less  than  that  of  the  self-pollinated 
trees.  The  hand  pollinated  results  in  the  tented  series  show  Wagener  to  be  an 
excellent  male  parent  and  a  percentage  fruit  of  47-11  was  obtained  from  indi- 
vidual hand  pollinated  limbs  on  the  untouched  trees  (i.e.  no  bees  and  no 
bouquets)  over  the  four-year  period.  On  the  open  pollinated  tree,  the  limb 
hand  pollinated  with  Wagener  only  gave  a  percentage  fruit  of  11-21.  This 
illustrates  the  fact  that  the  results  obtained  on  hand  pollinated  limbs  are  in 
direct  proportion  to  the  total  crop  borne  by  all  limbs  over  the  entire  tree. 


(vi)   Summary  of  Results  with  All  Varieties 

on 

Gravenstein  (1928-1932). 

Baldwin 

Cox  Orange 

Golden  Russet 

Total  blossoms    1      Per  cent  fruit 

Total  blossoms  |    Per  cent  fruit 

Total  blossoms  1  Per  cent  fruit 

4,854 

0-87 

5,441                       15-25 

5,104                        12-72 

King 

Mcintosh 

Wagener 

5,857 

I              1-52 

I 

5,030                       12  01 

5,876                       14  06 

The  triploid  varieties,  namely  Baldwin  and  King,  tested  on  GravensteiD 
gave  uniformly  poor  results  throughout,  indicating  the  inadvisability  of  inter- 
planting  Gravenstein  orchards  with  such  varieties.  All  diploids  tested  on  this 
variety  gave  excellent  results,  as  the  above  table  indicates,  and  the  varieties 
chosen  to  inter-plant  with  Gravenstein  depend  largely  on  individual  preference, 
location,  soil  type,  and  market  demand. 

(vii)  Other  Data. — Unfavourable  weather  prevailed  during  the  1931  sea- 
son. Three  of  the  hand  pollinated  limbs  in  the  tented  series  were  pollinated  on 
May  23,  the  remaining  three  on  May  25.  The  former  gave  an  average  per- 
centage fruit  of  27-85,  the  latter  8-60  per  cent.  The  preceding  figures  indicate 
that  a  higher  yield  is  obtained  if  pollination  is  carried  out  when  the  stigmas 
are  first  receptive  rather  than  near  the  end  of  stigma  receptivity.  That  is  to 
say,  pollination  results  appear  to  be  more  favourable  when  the  blossom  first 
opens.    This  result  was  consistent  with  other  varieties  observed. 

(viii)  General  Summary  for  Variety. — Gravenstein  is  the  most  self-unfruit- 
ful of  the  standard  varieties  grown  in  the  Annapolis  valley.  In  view  of  the 
fact  that  it  is  a  triploid,  it  makes  a  rather  poor  male  parent.  As  a  female 
parent,  this  variety  may  be  considered  good  and  when  such  pollinizers  as  Cox 
Orange,  Golden  Russet,  Mcintosh  and  Wagener,  all  diploids,  are  present,  excel- 
lent results  are  obtained. 

(e)  KING 

(i)  Results  of  Other  Workers. — Overholser  (1927 1  in  studies  in  California 
found  King  to  be  self-unfruitful.  Furthermore,  he  found  it  to  be  a  very  unsatis- 
factory male  parent,  being  similar  to  Baldwin  in  this  respect.     He  found  King 


59 


Fig.    16. — Photographs   illustrating  results   of   experimental    pollinations:     (1)    King 
selfed;    (2)   King  x  Wagener;    (3)   King  x  Baldwin    (original). 


60 

to  be  a  fair  female  parent,  with  Jonathan  proving  the  most  effective  pollinizer. 
Crane  and  Lawrence  (1929),  in  experiments  conducted  at  Merton,  England,  find 
King  to  be  partially  self-fruitful,  a  percentage  set  of  1-9  being  obtained.  Wel- 
lington, Stout,  et  at.  (1929),  found  this  variety  to  be  also  partially  self-fruitful 
in  New  York  state,  while  Auchter  (1921)  lists  it  as  self-fruitful  in  Maryland. 
Other  workers,  e.g.,  Morris  (1921);  Chittenden  (1926),  and  Lewis  and  Vin- 
cent (1909)  place  King  as  a  self-unfruitful  variety.  As  a  male  parent  the  above 
writers  find  it  poor.  Wellington,  Stout,  et  at.  (1929)  found  Delicious  to  be  an 
effective  pollinizer  for  King,  good  commercial  sets  resulting. 


(ii)  Results 

of  Selfing  Tests 

on  King  (1928- 

1932). 

Total  blossoms 

Total  set 

Per  cent  set 

Total  fruit 

Per  cent  fruit 

7,221 

1,072 

14-85 

257 

3-56 

The  results  over  five  years  indicate  that  King  is  fairly  self-fruitful,  selfing 
tests  giving  an  average  percentage  "set"  of  14-85  and  "fruit"  3-56.  This 
figure  may  even  be  considered  self-fruitful  for  the  variety,  because  of  the  fact 
that  the  open  pollinated  trees  over  the  period  tested  gave  only  4-74  per  cent 
fruit.  In  this  connection  also  should  be  considered  the  large  number  of  blos- 
soms produced  by  this  variety,  the  large  size  of  the  fruit  and  the  annual  bearing 
habit  of  the  King  in  many  commercial  orchards. 

(iii)  Results  from  King  as  Female  Parent  (1928-1932). 


Total  blossoms 

Total  set 

Per  cent  set 

Total  fruit 

Per  cent  fruit 

32,148 

6,648 

20-68 

1,549 

4-82 

King  as  a  female  parent  may  be  considered  good,  a  "set"  of  20-68  and 
"  fruit"  of  4-82  per  cent,  being  obtained  over  the  five-year  period.  The  above 
figure  is  extremely  good  when  one  takes  into  consideration  the  fact  that  between 
3-5  and  4  per  cent  fruit  on  a  heavy  blossoming  tree  will  result  in  a  good  com- 
mercial crop,  with  this  variety.  The  diploids  tested,  Cox  Orange,  Golden  Rus- 
set, Mcintosh  and  Wagener,  all  increased  the  "set"  and  "fruit"  percentage 
above  that  of  selfing  and  above  that  obtained  where  triploids  were  used.  It 
should  be  noted  that  a  small  increase  of  one  half  of  one  per  cent  is  of  great 
economic  importance  in  this  variety,  because  of  the  low  percentage  fruit  neces- 
sary to  give  a  satisfactory  crop. 

(iv)   Results  from  King  as  Male  Parent  (1928-1932). 


Total  blossoms 

Total  set 

Per  cent  set 

Total  fruit 

Per  rent  fruit 

Per  cent  seeds 

29,941 

3,458 

11-55 

1,068 

3-57 

11'  -40 

This  variety  may  be  considered  very  unsatisfactory  as  a  male  parent, 
being  similar  to  Baldwin  in  this  respect.  The  results  for  the  pasl  live  year- 
show  a  percentage  "set"  of  11-55,  "fruit"  of  3-57  and  seed,  12-40.  which  is 
quite  in  line  with  the  results  of  other  triploid  varieties. 

(v)  Evidence  from  Tented  Series. — The  tent  studies  indicate  that  King  is 
commercially  fairly  self-fruitful.  The  "untouched"  tests,  that  is.  the  tented 
trees  with  no  bees  and  no  bouquets,  gave  a  percentage  fruit  of  only  1-03.     The 


61 

open  pollinated,  with  a  percentage  fruit  of  4-74,  exceeds  the  selfed  trees  by  a 
little  over  one  per  cent,  the  latter  having  an  average  of  3-32  per  cent  fruit 
for  the  past  four  years.  In  the  cases  where  Wagener  bouquets  were  introduced 
as  a  means  of  cross-pollination,  the  per  cent  fruit  was  increased  nearly  one  per 
cent,  the  average  figure  being  5-42.  On  the  other  hand,  in  those  trees  where 
Blenheim  was  used,  the  per  cent  fruit  closely  approached  that  of  selfing,  namely 
3-58.  It  might  be  well  to  state  that  King  shows  less  difference  between  different 
male  parents  than  any  variety  tested,  a  habit  associated  with  the  very  low  per- 
centage fruit  to  spur  area.  Even  though  the  original  set  may  show  consider- 
able differences,  dropping  takes  place  until  the  margin  between  crossing  and 
selfing  results  is  greatly  reduced. 


(vi)  Summary  of  Results  with  All 

Varieties  on 

King   (1928-1932). 

Baldwin 

Cox  Orange 

Golden  Russet 

Total  blossoms 

Per  cent  fruit 

Total  blossoms 

Per  cent  fruit 

Total  blossoms 

Per  cent  fruit 

5,790 

4-27 

5,530 

5-08 

5,084 

4-33 

Gravenstein 

Mcintosh 

Wagener 

5,144 

4-08 

4,565           |            5-56 

1 

6,035            |            5-58 

I 

The  triploids,  namely,  Baldwin  and  Gravenstein,  gave  much  better  results 
on  this  variety  than  one  might  expect,  both  showing  an  increase  of  one-half 
of  one  per  cent  over  the  selfing  tests.  All  the  diploids,  Cox  Orange,  Golden 
Russet,  Mcintosh  and  Wagener,  gave  an  increase  of  one  per  cent  over  the 
selfed  tests  and  from  one-half  to  one  per  cent  over  the  triploids  used.  This 
increase  might  appear  of  little  importance  in  some  varieties,  but  in  the  case 
of  King,  with  its  normally  low  per  cent  of  set  to  bloom,  it  is  significant. 

(vii)  Other  Data. — The  results  obtained  during  1931  in  a  King  orchard  at 
Wolfville,  were  uniformly  lower  than  those  obtained  at  Kentville  in  1928,  1929, 
1930  and  1932.  This  may  be  explained  by  the  fact  that  the  trees  in  the  Wolf- 
ville orchard  are  in  an  annual-bearing  habit,  whereas  those  used  in  Kentville 
were  decidedly  biennial. 

(viii)  General  Summary  for  Variety. — In  summarizing  the  results  from  this 
variety  it  may  be  stated  that  King  is  fairly  self-fruitful  from  a  commercial 
standpoint.  As  a  male  parent  it  is  rather  poor,  being  next  in  line  to  Baldwin; 
as  a  female  parent  it  is  good,  when  one  takes  into  consideration  its  blossoming 
and  bearing  habits,  together  with  the  large  size  of  individual  fruits.  Triploid 
crosses  such  as  Baldwin  and  Gravenstein  do  not  depress  fruitfulness,  as  was 
found  to  be  the  case  when  used  with  most  other  varieties  tested,  and  all  the 
diploids  tested  showed  their  value  as  pollinizers  by  increasing  the  percentage 
fruit  to  bloom,  to  the  extent  of  one  per  cent  in  most  cases. 


(/)  NORTHERN  SPY 

(i)  MacDaniels  (1928)  in  experiments  in  New  York,  reports  Northern  Spy 
as  self-unfruitful  and  found  that,  where  pollen  was  not  applied  by  hand,  total 
crop  failure  followed.  Much  greater  variation  in  response  to  pollination  is  found 
in  trees  of  low  vigour  than  in  those  growing  under  better  conditions.  MacDaniels 
(loc.  cit.)  lists  the  suitable  pollinizers  for  Northern  Spy  as  Wealthy,  Golden 
Delicious,  Rome  Beauty,  N.W.  Greening,  Tolman  Sweet,  and  Delicious.  How- 
ever, because  of  the  late  blooming  habit  of  Spy,  Rome  is  the  only  dependable 
source  of  pollen.  Marshall,  Johnson,  et  al.  (1929),  in  Michigan,  find  Spy  self- 
unfruitful,  but  place  it  as  an  effective  pollinizer  and  as  a  good  female  parent. 


62 


Fig.  17-  Photographs  illustrating  results  of  experimental  pol- 
linations: (1)  Spy  self ed;  (2)  Spy  x  Golden  Russet;  (3)  Spy 
x  Baldwin   (original ) . 


63 

With  one  exception,  viz.,  Gowen  (1920),  who  obtained  results  indicating  partial 
self-fruitfulness,  all  investigators  term  Northern  Spy  self-unfruitful,  but  record 
the  variety  as  an  excellent  male  and  female  parent. 

(ii)  Results  of  Selfing  Tests  on  Spij   (1928-1932). 


Total  blossoms 

Total  set 

Per  cent  set 

Total  fruit 

Per  cent  fruit 

5,862 

249 

4-25 

78 

1  •  33 

The  hand  pollinated  results  at  Kentville  during  the  years  1928-1931,  inclu- 
sive, proved  the  variety  to  be  quite  self-unfruitful,  yielding  an  average  of  4-25 
per  cent  "set"  and  1-33  per  cent  "fruit".  This  figure  approaches  that 
secured  in  Gravenstein  selfing  tests,  and  may  be  considered  the  second  lowest, 
in  regard  to  self-unfruitfulness,  among  the  six  standard  varieties  under  test. 

(iii)   Results  from  Spy  as  Female  Parent   (1928-1932). 


Total  blossoms 

Total  set 

Per  cent  set 

Total  fruit 

Per  cent  fruit 

27,252 

7,467 

27-40 

2,673 

9-81 

Spy  is  an  excellent  female  parent,  27-40  per  cent  "set"  and  9-81  per 
cent  "  fruit "  being  obtained  over  the  period.  The  diploid  sorts  gave  very 
satisfactory  results  on  this  variety,  Ben  Davis  being  especially  good.  Triploids 
proved,  on  the  other  hand,  to  be  unsatisfactory  as  male  parents  for  Spy,  and 
in  many  cases  no  fruit  was  obtained.  Other  varieties  tested  at  the  Kentville 
station,  but  not  reported  in  this  paper,  are  Delicious,  Golden  Delicious  and 
Red  Rome  Beauty,  all  of  which  gave  good  results  and  all  overlap  the  Spy  in 
bloom  to  a  satisfactory  extent. 


(iv)   Results  from  Spy 

as  Male  Parent    (1928-1932). 

Total  blossoms 

Total  set 

Per  cent  set 

Total  fruit 

Per  cen  t  fruit 

Per  cent  seeds 

6,077 

2,838 

46-70 

756 

12-44 

54-08 

The  above  results,  as  to  the  value  of  Spy  as  a  male  parent,  are  based  entirely 
on  pollinations  made  on  the  Baldwin  variety,  and  from  these  it  may  be  con- 
sidered an  excellent  pollinizer  for  the  late  blooming  varieties.  Spy  was  also 
used  in  1928  and  1929  as  a  male  parent  for  Cox  Orange  and  Golden  Russet,  a 
per  cent  "fruit"   of   14-44   and   10-60  respectively   being  obtained. 

(v)  Evidence  from  Tented  Series.— The  results  from  the  tented  series  indi- 
cate that  Spy  is  quite  self-unfruitful,  a  percentage  "fruit"  of  2-00  being 
obtained  from  the  selfed  trees  (bees  and  no  bouquets)  in  the  past  four  years. 
The  untouched  trees  (no  bees  and  no  bouquets)  gave  a  percentage  fruit  of  0-85, 
for  the  same  period,  which  is  nearly  one  per  cent  lower  than  the  selfed  trees,  and 
shows  that  natural  selfing  in  the  absence  of  bees  is  very  low.  In  the  case  of  open 
pollination,  we  find  a  relatively  high  percentage  fruit  of  8-83  and,  where  an 
effective  pollinizer  was  used,  the  per  cent  fruit  was  10-05.     In  contrast  to  this, 


64 

where  an  ineffective  pollinizer  was  tried,  the  fruit  was  reduced  to  2-70  per  cent, 
almost  as  low  as  the  selfed  trees.  The  fact  that  triploids-  were  used  as  ineffective 
pollinizers  in  this  case  explains  the  low  fruit  percentage. 

(vi)  Summary  of  Results  with  All  Varieties  on  Spy  (1928-1932). 


Bald 

win 

Ben  Davis 

CoxC 

> range 

G.  Russet 

King 

Total 
blos- 
soms 

Per 

cent 
fruit 

Total 
blos- 
soms 

Per 

cenfc 
fruit 

Total 
blos- 
soms 

Per 

cent 
fruit 

Total 
blos- 
soms 

Per 

cent 
fruit 

Total 
blos- 
soms 

Per 
cent 
fruit 

4,488 

2-41 

4,785 

15-92 

5,843 

14-58 

6,142 

11-98 

5,994 

3-59 

The  triploids,  Baldwin  and  King,  when  used  as  male  parents  give  very 
unsatisfactory  results,  a  very  low  per  cent  fruit  resulting.  Ben  Davis,  Cox  Orange 
and  Golden  Russet  are  effective  pollinizers ;  the  first  of  these  is  most  outstanding, 
the  blooming  periods  overlapping  satisfactorily,  but  Cox  Orange  gives  fair 
results. 

(vii)  Other  Data. — Tests  conducted  in  an  orchard  at  Greenwich,  as  to  rate 
of  pollen  tube  growth  within  this  variety,  incidentally  showed  Ben  Davis,  Cox 
Orange  and  Golden  Russet  to  be  the  outstanding  pollinizers  under  test. 

(viii)  General  Summary  for  Variety. — We  may  consider  Spy  quite  self- 
unfruitful,  being  like  Gravenstein  in  this  respect.  It  is  excellent  as  a  male  and 
female  parent;  our  figures  show  it  to  be  superior  in  the  former  respect.  Ben  Davis 
and  Cox  Orange  are  the  only  suitable  pollinizers  reported  on.  Rome  Beauty  is  an 
excellent  male  parent  for  the  variety,  and  being  a  late  blossoming  variety  is  thus 
a  dependable  source  of  pollen,  but,  in  view  of  the  fact  that  it  has  little  economic 
importance  as  yet  in  the  Annapolis  valley,  no  tests  have  been  conducted  to 
ascertain  its  value  under  our  conditions. 

The  summarized  results  from  the  standpoint  of  "  set "  and  "  fruit "  are 
included  in  table  8  and  the  result  by  groups,  including  also  seed  data  in  table  9. 


TABLE  No.  8. 


-TOTAL  RESULTS  OF  APPLE  CROSSES  WITH  STANDARD  VARIETIES 
(1928-1932) 


Male  Parents 

Female  Parent 

Baldwin 

(  'ox 
Orange 

G  raven- 
stein 

Golden 
Russet 

King 

Spy 

Per 
cent 
set 

Per 

cent 
fruit 

Per 

cent 

set 

Per 
cent 

fruit 

Per 

cent 
set 

Per 
cent 
fruit 

Per 

cent 
set 

Per 
cent 

fruit 

Per 

cent 
set 

Per 

cent 
fruit 

Per 

cent 
set 

Per 

cent 

fruit. 

Baldwin 

Cox  Orange 

24-96 
1619 

8-42 

2-33 

18-89 

6-37 

6-37 
2-51 
233 

0-87 
4-27 
2-41 

48-46 
12-60 
37-67 
28-95 
18-26 
38-75 

14  -64 
1-81 
7-51 

15-25 
5-08 

14-58 

37-54 
19-86 
12-69 
2-43 
16-33 

8-93 
1-97 
2-28 
115 
408 

46  00 
49-39 
12-59 

32-86 
23-94 

38-62 

12-25 
13-99 

173 
12-72 

4-33 
11-98 

26-97 
11-72 
9-57 
2-78 
1  \  85 
7-84 

8-65 
2-47 

215 
l  52 

3-56 
3-59 

46-70 

12-44 

Golden  Russet 

Gravenstein 

King 

Spy 

4-25 

1-33 

65 

TABLE  No.  9.— THE  FRUITFULNESS  OF  DIFFERENT  TYPES  OF  CROSSES  (1928-1932) 


Cross 


Total 
blossoms 


Total 
set 


Per 

cent 
set 


Total 
fruit 


Per 
cent 
fruit 


Seeds" 


Per 

cent 


Aver- 
age 
number 
per  fruit 


Diploid  x  Diploid 

Diploid  x  Triploid 

Diploid  Selfed 

Triploid  x  Diploid 

Triploid  x  Triploid 

Triploid  x  Triploid  (without  Baldwin  as 

Female  Parent) 

Triploid  Selfed 


43,294 
46,426 
22,894 
60,284 
32,267 

21,645 
22,021 


17,790 
5,538 
2,394 

19,573 
5,623 

2,210 
2,976 


41-09 
11-93 
10-46 
32-47 
17-43 

10-21 
13-51 


5,242 
1,342 
380 
6,308 
1,521 

588 
785 


12-11 
2-89 
1-66 

10-46 
4-71 

2-72 
3-56 


75-70 

9-93 

6-38 

36-79 

13-49 

6-81 
9  04 


"Seed  counts  made  on  basis  of  actual  number  of  fruit  harvested. 


(h)  SEED   CONTENT   IN   RELATION   TO   FRUITFULNESS 

Pollen  of  high  germinability  and  good  quality  for  crossing  together  with 
relatively  high  seed  content  is  associated  with  diploidy  in  apples,  the  reverse  with 
triploidy.  Diploid  varieties,  as  female  parents,  have  consistently  given  a  higher 
seed  content  than  triploid  varieties  and  within  the  variety  the  seed  content  is 
affected  by  the  male  parent,  diploids  giving  more  seeds  than  triploids  in  this 
respect.  Although  diploid  x  diploid  and  triploid  x  diploid  crosses  are  of  approx- 
imately equal  value  as  to  fruitfulness,  the  latter  has  a  much  lower  average  seed 
content.  Furthermore,  though  triploid  crosses  often  exceed  diploid  x  diploid  in 
fruitfulness,  the  latter  produced  a  higher  average  seed  content  in  these  tests. 
Percentage  fruitfulness  is  generally  proportional  to  seed  content  but  is  not  neces- 
sarily directly  so,  as  explained  above. 

In  seed  germination,  triploids  as  a  group  show  a  much  lower  germination 
than  diploids,  i.e.,  when  used  as  females;  on  the  other  hand  the  male  parent  has 
no  measurable  effect  on  germination. 

The  results  from  the  standpoint  of  viable  seedlings  resulting  from  the  fore- 
going types  of  crosses,  are  consistent  with  those  obtained  with  seed.  The  order 
of  seedling  production  was  the  same,  viz.:  (1)  diploid  x  diploid,  (2)  triploid  x 
diploid,  (3)  diploid  x  triploid  and  (4)  triploid  x  triploid.  Details  are  discussed 
in  another  paper. 

(0  RELATION   OF   SEED   CONTENT  TO   WEIGHT 

As  already  indicated,  many  workers  have  claimed  a  correlation  between 
weight  and  seed  content  in  the  apple.  The  fact  that  one-sided  apples  show  some 
of  the  carpels  empty  on  the  corresponding  side  is  a  matter  of  general  observa- 
tion. Samples  picked  at  random  offer  little  evidence  in  this  connection,  since 
many  factors  influence  size  and  weight  of  fruit,  and  a  disturbing  factor  is  intro- 
duced in  the  utilization  of  fruits  resulting  from  mixed  pollination.  On  the  other 
hand,  trees  with  a  very  low  set  due  to  an  unfruitful  cross  produce  few  apples, 
and  those  that  do  set  may  grow  abnormally  large  due  to  favourable  nutritional 
conditions.  For  this  reason,  it  appears  desirable  that  the  samples  selected  should 
be  produced  under  uniform  and  normal  conditions.  For  the  foregoing  reason  in 
1931  we  selected  two  varieties,  Gravenstein,  as  representative  of  a  triploid 
variety  with  very  low  seed  content,  and  Northern  Spy,  representative  of  a  diploid 
variety  with  an  exceptionally  high  seed  content.  For  our  study  we  selected  a 
tented  tree  of  each  variety  which  had  been  provided  with  a  hive  of  bees  and  an 
effective  pollinizer,  Wagener  in  the  case  of  Gravenstein,  Ben  Davis  in  the  case  of 

60796—5 


66 

Spy.  All  the  apples  on  each  tree  were  taken,  500  in  the  case  of  Gravenstein 
and  1 ,596  in  the  case  of  Spy.  By  thus  providing  optimum  conditions  for  pollina- 
tion we  naturally  reduced  the  production  of  abnormal  apples  likely  to  result  from 
imperfect  fertilization,  which  undoubtedly  affected  the  results,  but  gave  a  value 
for  the  effect  of  seed  content. 


Fig.  18. — Studies  in  premature  drop  and  open  "blossom  end" 
of  Gravenstein:  (l)-(5)  closed  "blossom  end",  no  mouldy 
core;  (6) -(10)  open  blossom  end,  mouldy  core;  (1)  8 
seeds;  (2)  6  developed  and  2  undeveloped  seeds;  (3)  5 
developed  and  2  undeveloped  seeds;  (4)  5  seeds;  (6)  1 
seed;  (7)  3  seeds;  (8)  3  seeds;  (9)  2  developed  and  1 
undeveloped  seed;    (10)  no  seeds  (original). 


67 

The  coefficient  of  correlation  using  all  seeds,  whether  filled  or  not,  was,  in 
the  case  of  Gravenstein  -055±  -0366,  which  is  not  significant;  and  for  Spy, 
•3467±  -0148,  which  is  statistically  just  significant.  Since  results  with  Graven- 
stein are  not  in  line  with  those  obtained  by  Einset  Hoc.  cit.)  and  since  the 
correlation  in  the  case  of  Spy  is  not  as  great  nor  as  striking  as  might  have 
been  expected,  it  was  decided  to  duplicate  the  work  with  Gravenstein  and  Spy 
and,  in  addition,  to  run  similar  correlations  with  King  pollinated  with  Wagener 
and  Baldwin  pollinated  with  Cox  Orange.  The  following  numbers  of  fruits 
examined  were:  Gravenstein,  1,100;  Spy,  1,000;  King,  314,  and  Baldwin,  1,000. 


8 


Fig. 


19.- — Outlines  of  "blossom  end"  of  Gravenstein  apples  showing  gradation   in  open 
condition   (original). 


The  coefficients  of  correlation  obtained  in  1932  are  as  follows:  Gravenstein, 
•0025±  -0302;  King,  -1103±  -0557;  Baldwin,  -2723±  -0293;  Spy,  -3069± 
•0286.  In  the  Gravenstein,  King  and  Baldwin  varieties,  no  significant  correla- 
tion was  obtained,  but  in  the  case  of  the  Spy  the  result  may  be  considered  just 
significant. 

In  order  to  determine  whether  chromosome  constitution  had  a  bearing  on 
this  phenomenon,  another  diploid,  viz.,  Wagener  was  selected  and  a  correlation 
calculated  on  one  thousand  apples  from  open  pollinated  trees.  The  correlation 
obtained  was  -07304  and  -0315,  which  was  not  significant.  It  may  be  noted 
that  the  average  number  of  seeds  obtained  for  Wagener,  viz.,  5-58,  is  no  greater 
than  for  Baldwin  and  certain  other  triploid  varieties. 


60796—51 


68 
\j)  MORPHOLOGICAL    ABNORMALITIES    ASSOCIATED    WITH   SEED    CONTENT 

In  varieties  with  a  normally  high  seed  content  the  failure  of  seed  on  one  side 
to  develop  results  in  the  fruit  being  flattened  on  the  corresponding  side.  Deter- 
minations of  seed  content  from  normal  and  one-sided  apples  of  the  same  variety 
show  a  higher  average  seed  content  and  a  correspondingly  greater  weight  in  the 
former.  Varieties  in  which  this  condition  was  commonly  observed  are:  Spy, 
Yellow  Bellflower  and  Ben  Davis.  This  type  of  distortion  seems  to  be  somewhat 
more  common  in  apples  of  an  elongate  form  than  in  those  that  are  more  flattened 
at  the  extremities.  In  certain  other  varieties,  apples  with  few  or  no  seeds  may 
be  found,  which  are  almost  cylindrical  in  shape.  Such  was  the  case  with  Deacon 
Jones,  in  which  type  the  number  of  seeds  is  twenty,  the  average  from  mixed 
samples  being  9-36. 

On  the  other  hand,  apples  with  a  relatively  low  average  seed  content  may 
show  no  distortion  whatever.  In  the  Wageners  examined  by  us  in  our  studies  of 
weight  in  relation  to  seed  content,  no  one-sided  specimens  were  found.  The 
same  is  true  of  all  the  triploid  varieties  studied,  viz.,  King,  Baldwin  and 
Gravenstein. 


/m?/arfr 


CoxChwye 


£i/ssef 


/f//7# 


J/art 


£/e/7few 


/kr/afa//? 


Fig.  20. — Outline  of  "blossom  end"  of  standard  varieties   (original). 


69 

It  has  already  been  noted  in  preceding  sections  that  few-seeded  and  even 
seedless  Gravensteins  may  be  normal  in  size,  weight  and  outer  form.  How- 
ever, such  apples  may  be  characterized  by  abnormalities  at  the  calyx  end,  which 
result  in  an  open  core  condition,  resulting  in  the  invasion  of  this  region  by 
saprophytic  fungi,  producing  what  is  known  as  "  mouldy  core."  The  late 
summer  drop  of  Gravenstein,  which  occurs  in  some  seasons,  is  also  correlated 
with  this  condition.  In  experiments  under  tents  it  was  found  that  trees  sup- 
plied with  a  suitable  source  of  cross-fruitful  pollen  and  an  adequate  force  of 
insect  pollinators  gave  approximately  one  half  the  amount  of  premature  dropped 
fruit.  Furthermore,  along  with  a  higher  seed  content,  effectively  pollinated 
apples  developed  a  negligible  percentage  of  open  calyx  end  and  "  mouldy  core." 
Ineffectively  pollinated  trees  produced  fruit  with  a  relatively  low  seed  content 
and  high  percentage  of  open  calyx  end  and  "  mouldy  core."  On  the  other  hand, 
the  open  pollinated  trees,  where  a  measure  of  effective  pollination  had  taken 
place,  were  intermediate  in  position. 

This  open  blossom-end  condition  has  also  been  noted  in  two  other  varieties, 
viz.,  Boskoop  and  Bramley  Seedling.  The  average  seed  content  of  these  varieties 
is  low,  approximating  that  of  Gravenstein.  In  addition,  a  single  tree  of  an 
unnamed  variety,  probably  a  seedling,  was  discovered  in  which  few  apples 
containing  seeds  could  be  found.  On  cutting  open  these  apples,  practically  all 
exhibited  this  condition.  The  foregoing  observations  are  based  mainly  upon 
preliminary  studies  made  in  1932.  No  extensive  survey  of  apple  varieties  to 
determine  the  nature  and  distribution  of  the  foregoing  abnormalities  has  been 
made,  and  much  further  research  is  needed  before  definite  conclusions  can  be 
drawn. 

5.  SUMMARY,    INTER-FRUITFULNESS    OF    APPLE    VARIETIES 

(a)  POLLEN  STUDIES 

1.  Technique  followed  in  pollen  tests: — 

(a)  All  pollen  was  gathered  from  nondehisced  anthers. 

(b)  It  was  matured  under  ideal  artificial  conditions. 

(c)  It  was  tested  in  sugar-agar  germinative  media. 

2.  The  original  medium  was  a  10  per  cent  cane  sugar  solution.  However,  it 
was  found  more  effective  to  use  a  medium  containing  15  per  cent  cane  sugar 
and  -5  per  cent  agar. 

3.  Counts  of  approximately  1,000  pollen  grains  were  made  for  each  variety. 

4.  Pollen  germination  tests  were  conducted  on  all  available  varieties. 

5.  The  results  obtained  with  the  different  varieties  from  year  to  year  were 
not  entirely  consistent,  but,  in  considering  each  year's  germination  tests,  the 
effective  pollinizers  gave  consistently  higher  germination  than  the  ineffective 
pollinizers. 

6.  An  exception  to  the  above  was  Stayman  Winesap  which,  although  germ- 
inating well  proved  ineffective  as  a  pollinizer,  due  to  an  agglomeration  of  pollen 
grains. 

7.  All  pollen  used  on  the  hand  pollinated  series  was  tested  for  effective 
germination  before  use. 

(b)  TENT  STUDIES 

1.  In  tent  studies,  cages  were  erected  over  trees  of  four  varieties,  viz.,  Bald- 
win, Gravenstein,  King,  and  Spy,  each  being  subdivided  into  the  following 
series: — 

1.  Tented,  with  bees  and  an  effective  pollinizer. 

2.  Tented,  with  bees  and  an  ineffective  pollinizer. 

3.  Tented,  with  bees  and  no  pollinizer. 


70 

4.  Tented,  with  no  bees  and  no  pollinizer. 

5.  Tented,  with  no  bees  but  with  effective  pollinizer. 

6.  Check  tree,  untented  and  left  open  pollinated,  abundance  of  pollin- 

ating insects  being  present. 

2.  The  method  followed  was  to  place  bees  and  bouquets  in  required  tents, 
without  emasculation  of  the  blossoms.  Results  in  emasculation  tests  show  that 
this  method  of  procedure  was  warranted. 


Se/feaf  /7jf. 
\A'//7a2/sf° 
I  Graves7.5/ie//7  2  28% 
\3a/aW//7  233% 

\/fc//7/as/?  706% 
I  Cox  Orar?ae  7f/f» 


\5e/fea '6J7f* 

\/<mo  86S7> 
Orarens/e//?  &  9J% 

Go/cSe/7  Xusse/  /2  ZS% 
\Spi/  /Z4-*Y. 

I  Cox  Ora/?ae  /464 

'/g/fc 

\/</S7(?  24? 'X 

\£a/a*'/r//r  2S/%" 

I  Crarerts/e/sr  -4 '  97jC 


Se/fea'JSO'f* 
Crarff/vs/e/s?  -403^ 
3/e/?/?e//7?  -40d% 
3a/a,/r//?427f<, 

Cox  Orar??*  SOd% 
SfcMos/r  SS6/0 


/099/. 

Oo/e/e/7 rfvsse/ '/J 99^ 


Cox  On7s7<?e  X4  S8% 
fiesr^ar/s  /S92% 

Fig.  21. — Diagram  showing  value  of  different  pollens  on 
standard  varieties    (original). 

3.  Bagged  limbs  used  as  checks  on  effectiveness  of  treatment  gave  con- 
sistent results,  which  further  verifies  the  foregoing  point. 

4.  The  exclusion  of  pollinating  insects  reduced  the  crop  produced  to  an 
unprofitable  level  in  all  cases. 

5.  In  all  cases,  the  use  of  an  effective  pollinizer  has  given  a  higher  percent- 
age fruit  than  where  ineffective  pollinizers  were  used. 

6.  The  introduction  of  an  effective  pollinizer  in  Baldwin,  Gravenstein,  King 
and  Spy  has  given  an  increase  in  yields  over  selfed  trees. 

7.  In  the  case  of  Baldwin,  a  good  commercial  crop  can  be  obtained  by 
selfing. 

8.  Gravenstein  and  Spy  are  self-unfruitful,  i.e.  commercial  crops  cannot  be 
obtained  through  selfing. 

9.  King  is  quite  self-fruitful,  giving  in  some  years  satisfactory  results  through 
selfing. 

10.  Wind  pollination  within  tented  trees  is  practically  nil. 

(c)  HAND  POLLINATION  STUDIES 

1.  To  secure  a  large  population  the  limb  unit  method  was  followed 

2.  Emasculation  was  not  practised,  but  large  populations  and  ext inane  care 
in  pollinating  before  the  anthers  had  dehisced,  more  than  compensated  for  the 
small  population  that  would  have  been  necessitated  had  emasculation  tech- 
nique been  practised. 


o 

series 

4 
5 


71 


General  results  with  this  series  are  in  agreement  with  those  of  the  tented 


Baldwin  and  King  are  self-fruitful,  the  former  to  a  greater  degree. 

No  one  variety  was  found  to  be  absolutely  self-unfruitful.  Cox  Orange, 
Golden  Russet,  Gravenstein  and  Spy  all  exhibited  self-fruitfulness  to  a  slight 
degree.    The  former  two  varieties  were  superior  in  this  respect  to  the  latter  two. 


P<ssu//s  of£>o///na//o/T 
w/tf?  D/enhe/m 


1930  1931  1932  J928  1923  1930 

Percent  frv/f  Od/a/nea 'ty  y&airs 


Fig.  22. — Graphs  showing  value  of  different  pollens  on  Spy, 
Blenheim.  Stark  and  Gravenstein   (original). 


6.  As  male  parents,  the  diploids,  namely,  Cox  Orange,  Golden  Russet  and 
Spy  proved  excellent.  On  the  other  hand,  the  triploids,  viz.,  Baldwin,  Graven- 
stein and  King  gave  uniformly  poor  results,  Gravenstein  being  superior  in  this 
respect  to  the  other  triploids. 

7.  As  female  parents,  Baldwin,  Cox  Orange,  Gravenstein,  King  and  Spy  are 
good.  Golden  Russet's  value  is  of  a  variable  nature,  tending  to  be  poor  on  the 
lighter  types  of  soils  and  good  on  the  heavier. 


72 


8.  The   following   classification   of  the   standard   varieties   shows  the   com- 
binations found  suitable  for  interplanting  in  the  Annapolis  valley: — 
Female  Male 

Baldwin  x  Cox  Orange  or  Spy. 

Cox  Orange  x  Golden  Russet,  Mcintosh  and  Wagener. 

Golden  Russet     x  Cox  Orange  or  Mcintosh. 

Gravenstein  x  Cox  Orange,  Golden  Russet,  Mcintosh  and  Wagener 

King  x  Cox  Orange,  Golden  Russet,  Mcintosh  and  Wagener 

Spy  x  Ben  Davis  and  Cox  Orange. 


Resu/fc  of '  Po////?a//o/7 


fte$u/te  of '  Po////?af/o/?  /y///? 
f<//?a 


6rayes?s/e//7 
Cox  Orange 


— —  Oo/aes?  ftusser 


■  6raye/75fe//t 
*Cox  Orange 


Oo/o'en  Ki/sse/ 


rv///?  3a/jjv//? 


■  Gra ye/75 /<?//? 


— ^6o/o/e/7  Kusset 

^Cox  Orange 

-  3a/aW/A 

/<//7t? 


rfesu/f?  0/ '  Po////7af/o/7 
jy/f/7  Cox  Orange  \ 

Gra/ensfe/n X/ng 

Cox  Orange .  -Ba/arr//? 


1929  1930  1931  193a  1926  1929  1930  1931  1932 

Percent  fnt/r  06fa//7ect  6y  Years 

Fig.  23.— Graphs  showing  value  of  different   pollens  on  Golden  Russet,  King,  Baldwin  ami  Cox  Oi 

(original). 


73 

6.    GENERAL    RESULTS    AND    CONCLUSIONS    FROM    STUDIES    IN 
INTER-FRUITFULNESS 

(a)  Owing  to  the  mixed  condition  of  Nova  Scotia  orchards  the  pollination 
problem  is  not  as  acute  as  in  some  fruitgrowing  districts.  Nevertheless,  clear 
evidence  of  unfruitfulness  from  the  practice  of  (1)  planting  self-unfruitful  varieties- 
in  blocks,  (2)  planting  cross-unfruitful  varieties  together  or  (3)  making  inade- 
quate  provision  for  c^oss-pollination,  has  been  obtained.  With  the  modern 
movement  to  restrict  new  plantings  to  a  smaller  number  of  commercially  desir- 
able sorts  and  to  cut  down  the  number  of  existing  varieties  by  "  grafting-out  " 
operations,  a  consideration  of  the  pollination  situation  becomes  increasingly 
important. 

(b)  The  detailed  results  of  hand  pollination  tests  with  standard  varieties 
are  shown  in  table  8,  and  the  group  results  for  the  different  types  of  crosses  in 
table  9.  A  study  of  the  results  which  are  based  on  very  large  populations,  indi- 
cates that  the  pollination  problem  is,  in  essence,  a  simple  one. 

(c)  The  value  of  a  variety  as  a  male  parent  is  closely  associated  with  the 
chromosome  constitution  of  the  variety.  All  diploid  varieties  tested  gave  good 
results  as  male  parents  for  other  varieties,  whether  diploids  or  triploids,  while 
triploid  varieties  gave  relatively  poor  results.  When  large  averages  are  con- 
sidered, there  is  little  significant  difference  between  the  different  diploids  tested, 
and,  provided  there  is  the  proper  degree  of  synchronization  between  pollen  libera- 
tion and  stigma  receptivity  in  the  varieties  concerned,  all  diploids  may  be 
expected,  on  the  basis  of  these  tests,  to  do  about  equally  well,  but  triploids 
appear  to  be  of  unequal,  though  inferior,  value.  Exceptions  to  this  general  rule 
may  occur  as  a  result  of  certain  mechanical  factors,  but  the  foregoing  appears 
to  be  true  for  the  varieties  tested  by  us.  Correlated  with  their  production  of  a. 
relatively  large  percentage  of  fruit  when  used  as  a  male  parent,  a  relatively  high 
percentage  pollen  germination  is  characteristic  of  diploid  varieties.  On  the  other 
hand,  a  relatively  low  pollen  germination  is  characteristic  of  triploid  sorts,  but 
pollen  germination  alone  does  not  account  for  the  results  secured.  They  a,re 
the  result  of  irregular  chromosome  distribution,  which  may  show  its  effect  either 
in  preventing  germination  or  in  causing  faulty  development  or  abortion  later. 

(d)  On  the  basis  of  results  obtained  with  the  varieties  tested,  there  would 
appear  to  be  little  significant  difference  between  the  diploid  x  diploid  type  of  cross 
and  the  triploid  x  diploid,  both  being,  to  a  high  degree,  fruitful.  On  the  other 
hand,  diploid  x  triploid  and  triploid  x  triploid  crosses,  excluding  Baldwin  as  a. 
female  parent  owing  to  possible  selfing,  are  both  very  unfruitful,  the  difference 
between  these  two  groups  in  our  tests  being  insignificant. 

(e)  It  is  by  no  means  certain,  however,  that  the  higher  average  fruitfulness- 
obtained  by  the  triploid  x  triploid  crosses  when  results  from  Baldwin  are  included, 
are  due  to  selfing  in  the  latter  variety.  All  triploids,  with  the  exception  of  Blen- 
heim, when  used  as  pollen  parents  for  this  variety  gave  average  results  superior 
to  selfing,  R.  I.  Greening  conspicuously  so.  They  are  also  higher  than  diploid 
x  triploid  crosses  which  may  be  due  to  the  triploid  x  triploid  cross  allowing; 
an  opportunity  lacking  in  diploid  x  triploid  crosses  for  the  union  of  diploid  or 
near  diploid  gametes. 

(/)  Diploid  varieties,  as  female  parents,  have  a  consistently  higher  seed  con- 
tent than  triploid  varieties.  With  diploid  varieties  the  seed  content  is  affected  by 
the  chromosome  constitution  of  the  male  parent,  diploids,  as  male  parents,  giving 
a  consistently  higher  seed  content  than  triploids. 

(g)  The  value  of  the  different  varieties  as  female  parents,  or  from  the  stand- 
point of  selfing,  is  not  so  clearly  associated  with  the  chromosome  number.  Fruit- 
ful and  relatively  unfruitful  sorts  are  found  among  both  diploids  and  triploids. 
Self-fruitfulness  is  found  in  varying  degrees  in  both  groups.   Of  all  the  varieties 


74 

tested,  Baldwin  is  the  most  self-fruitful.  King  is  fairly  self-fruitful,  but  such 
triploid  varieties  as  Gravenstein  and  Blenheim  are,  ordinarily,  conspicuously  self- 
unfruitful.  The  results  indicate  the  variable  character  of  self-fruitfulness  from 
year  to  vear  and  under  different  conditions. 

(h)  Not  only  is  the  value  of  the  male  parents  indicated  by  the  seed  con- 
tent of  the  female  parent  upon  which  their  pollen  is  used,  but  the  percentage 
of  seedlings  resulting  when  such  seed  is  planted,  affords  evidence  in  the  same 
direction,  which  is  presented  in  another  paper  published  elsewhere. 

(i)  No  correlation  between  seed  content  and  weight  could  be  demonstrated 
for  Gravenstein,  King,  Baldwin  and  Wagener.    Spy  showed  a  slight  correlation. 

(;)  Malformation  of  fruit  in  the  form  of  one-sided  or  cylindrically-shaped 
apples  results  from  imperfect  fertilization  in  certain  varieties,  chiefly  those  with 
a  relatively  high  average  seed  content. 

{k)  In  certain  other  varieties  with  a  low  average  seed  content,  a  condition 
known  as  "  open  blossom  end  "  results  when  the  seed  content  is  below  normal 
as  a  result  of  poor  pollination.  In  Gravenstein,  imperfect  pollination,  "  open 
blossom  end  "  and  "  mouldy  core  "  were  found  to  be  associated. 

7.     POLLINATION  TESTS  WITH   BLENHEIM   AND   STARK 
(a)     THE  PROBLEM 

Of  all  the  varieties  grown  in  the  Annapolis  valley,  the  most  frequent  com- 
plaints of  lack  of  fruitfulness  are  heard  regarding  Blenheim  and  Stark.  The 
complaint  regarding  Blenheim  comes  mainly  from  Kings  county  where  many 
growers  report  unsatisfactory  yields.  Attempts  to  correlate  this  failure  with 
cultural  or  nutritional  factors,  gave  inconclusive  results.  The  type  of  soil  was 
thought  to  have  an  influence,  since  in  some  cases  plantings  on  the  same  farm 
on  light  and  on  heavy  soil,  seemed  to  favour  the  latter,  but  important  excep- 
tions were  noted.  The  greatest  trouble  appeared  to  exist  where  large  blocks 
were  planted  together  or  where  there  were  indications  that  cross-unfruitful 
varieties  might  be  responsible.  In  some  cases  where  satisfactory  crops  were 
obtained,  it  appeared  to  be  generally  true  that  they  were  mixed  with  other 
varieties,  but  under  very  similar  conditions  other  trees  did  not  bear.  In  one 
orchard  where  Cox  Orange  had  been  grafted  into  the  tops  of  the  trees  no  benefit 
resulted. 

Many  hand  pollinations  were  made  on  individual  limbs  on  such  non-bearing 
trees,  and  in  all  cases  the  results  were  striking  and  even  spectacular  where 
diploids  were  used  as  male  parents.  In  one  orchard  at  Lakeville  in  1929,  limbs 
crossed  with  Cox  Orange  gave  a  percentage  fruit  of  23,  20,  36,  28  and  21  respec- 
tively, as  compared  with  2-25,  1-09,  0-44  and  0  when  selfed,  and  1-3,  2-52. 
0-55,  0-52  and  2-09  when  open  pollinated.  Many  similar  results  were  secured. 
From  these  it  would  seem  that  pollination  might  be  a  factor,  though  it  did  not 
appear  to  be  the  sole  factor  in  all  cases. 

Complaints  regarding  Stark  were  only  less  frequent  than  those  regarding 
Blenheim  and  the  trouble  appeared  to  be  even  more  widespread.  In  the  case 
of  Stark  the  chief  complaint  was  of  its  erratic  and  unreliable  bearing  habits. 
As  with  Blenheim  the  most  trouble  came  from  solid  plantings  of  the  variety,  or 
when  it  was  mixed  with  triploid  sorts.  Hand  pollination  tests  with  cross-fruitful 
varieties  gave  similar  though  perhaps,  less  spectacular  results  than  in  the  case 
of  Blenheim.  There  likewise  seemed  to  be  an  indication  that  lack  of  pollination 
might  be  responsible  for  at  least  a  great  deal  of  the  trouble  with  this  variety. 

It  was  therefore  decided  to  make  extensive  tests  using  a  large  number  of 
varieties,  mainly  to  determine  what  were  the  most  effective  pollinizers  for  these 
two  varieties.  The  results  of  1928-1930,  which  were  obtained  by  the  spur  unit 
method,  were  averaged  with  those  of  the  year  1931,  in  which  the  limb  unit 
method  was  used. 


Fig.  24. — Photographs  illustrating  results  of  experimental  pollinations: 
(1)  Blenheim  selfed;  (2)  Blenheim  x  Wagener;  (3)  Blenheim  x 
Stark    (original). 


76 

(6)     BLENHEIM 

(i)   Results  of  Other  Workers. — Little  information  appears  to  be  available* 
regarding  this  variety.    It  has  been  recorded  as  self-unfruitful  by  Harper  (1921) 
and  as  partially  self-fruitful,  yielding  0-93  per  cent  fruit  when  selfed,  by  Crane- 
and  Lawrence  (1929).     The  latter  authors  classify  this  variety  as  a  triploid. 

(ii)   Results  of  Selfing  Tests  on  Blenheim  (1928-1932). 


Total   blossoms 

Total  set 

Per  cent  set 

Total  fruit 

Per  cent  fruit 

5,279 

124 

2-35 

49 

0-93 

The  results  of  selfing  tests  at  Kentville  over  the  1928-1932  period,  show  a 
percentage  "  set  "  of  2-35  and  a  percentage  "  fruit  "  of  0-93,  indicating  definite 
self-unf  rait  fulness.  In  1931,  the  average  per  cent  "  fruit  "  on  the  selfed  limbs 
was  5-23,  which  is  a  remarkable  increase  and  further  illustrates  the  fact  that 
self-fruitfulness  is  not  a  fixed  character,  but  varies  with  the  factors  affecting 
fruit-bud  formation,  spur  vigour,  pollen  germination,  pollen  tube  growth  and 
possibly  other  factors.  On  the  whole,  it  would  appear  that  this  variety  is  one 
of  the  most  self-unfruitful  sorts,  and  good  results  should  not  be  expected  in 
ordinary  seasons  from  planting  solid  blocks  of  this  variety. 

(iii)   Results  of  Blenheim  as  Female  Parent  (1928-1932). 


Total  blossoms. 

Total  set 

Per  cent  set 

Total  fruit 

Per  cent  fruit 

30,255 

5,788 

1913 

2,573 

8-50 

Our  figures  represent  a  very  large  total  of  blossoms,  viz.,  30,255.  used  in 
crossing  tests  with  this  variety  and  with  many  different  male  parents.  The 
majority  of  these,  however,  were  selected  on  the  basis  of  their  high  pollen  germ- 
ination, as  reported  by  other  workers,  and  uniformly  good  results  might,  on 
the  basis  of  results  of  other  work,  be  expected.  The  foregoing  tests,  over  a 
five-year  period,  gave  a  percentage  "set"  of  19-13  and  a  percentage  "  fruit ■ "' 
of  8-50,  which  indicates  that  Blenheim  is  very  fruitful  as  a  female  parent,  where 
effective  pollinizers  are  used.  The  open  pollinated  trees  (three-year  average, 
1930-1932)  gave  a  percentage  "  fruit  "  of  7-92  for  this  variety,  which  show- 
clearly  that  the  value  indicated  may  be  a  little  higher  than  average. 


(iv)  Result 

s  of  Blenheim  as  Male  Parent  (1928-1932). 

Total  blossoms 

Total  set 

Per  cent  set 

Total  fruit 

Per  cent   fruit 

Per  cent  Beeds. 

2,808 

259 

9-22 

82 

2-92 

19-25 

Our  figures  for  this  variety  as  a  male  parent  are  based  only  on  2.808  blos- 
soms, and  confined  to  tests  on  Stark.  The  above  tests  -how  a  percentage 
"  fruit  "  of  2-92,  and  "  set  "  of  9-22,  which  is  lower  than  Stark  selfed,  indicating 
cross-unfruitfulness  and  may  be  taken  as  indicative  of  its  low  value  as  a  male 
parent,  a  fact  that  is  substantiated  by  other  tests. 


77 


(v)   Summary  of  Results  of  All  Varieties  on  Blenheim  (1928-1932) 


Male  Parent 


Baldwin 

Ben  Davis 

Bishop  Pippin 

Bough  Sweet 

fox  Orange 

Crimson  Beauty — 

Delicious 

Duchess 

Fallawater 

Fameuse 

Golden  Russet 

Gravenstein 

Grimes  Golden 

Hubbardston 

Jonathan 

King 

Mcintosh 

Melba 

Nonpareil 

Ontario 

Red  Astrachan 

R.  I.  Greening 

Ribston 

Rome  Beauty 

Spy 

Stark 

Stayman  Winesap.. . 

Wagener 

Wealthy 

Wellington 

Winter  Banana 

Wolf  River 

Yellow  Transparent 
York  Imperial 


Total 
blossoms 


371 
566 
431 
101 

4,280 
220 
993 
614 
375 
344 

2,000 

2,345 
323 
366 
885 

2,437 
895 
634 
491 
441 
637 
405 
541 
127 
144 

1,900 
630 

2,783 

1,302 
326 
343 
597 

1,040 
440 


Per  cent 

set 


10-78 
27-56 
33-64 

2-97 
24-69 
15-91 
25-28 
28-34 

6-93 
19-48 
26-30 
10-70 
26-32 
1612 
20-90 

4-60 
19-66 
21-61 

6-31 
26-30 
2418 

8-15 

9-61 
71-65 
22-92 

4-42 
11-75 
20  01 
15-75 
34-66 
29-15 
29-98 
35-48 
29-55 


Per  cent 
fruit 


It  is  hardly  necessary  to  point  out  that  where  crosses  represent  a  very  small 
blossom  population,  irregularities  due  to  limb  vigour,  pollen  germination,  etc., 
greatly  increase  the  probability  of  error  in  the  results. 

(vi)  Other  Data. — As  already  indicated,  data  from  commercial  orchards 
and  hand  pollination  tests  seem  to  show  that,  where  effectively  pollinated,  Blen- 
heim is  capable  of  producing  good  crops,  though  it  would  seem  that  some  other 
factor  must  be  involved  in  at  least  some  cases  of  non-bearing,  or  that  the  variety 
experiences  more  difficulty  in  becoming  cross-pollinated  than  many  others.  Its 
pollen  has  a  low  value  for  crossing  purposes  and  its  self-unfruitfulness  is  most 
pronounced.  In  1931  it  gave  a  high  percentage  "  fruit "  in  selfing  tests  and  this 
coincided  with  a  heavy  yield  of  Blenheims  all  over  the  valley.  Many,  though 
not  all,  plantings  that  had  yielded  poor  crops  before,  gave  good  yields  in  1931. 
Nothing  could  better  illustrate  the  variable  character  of  the  factor  of  self- 
fruitfulness. 

This  point  is  well  illustrated  by  an  experiment  conducted  at  Watervillc. 
There  was  a  block  of  Blenheim  that  the  owner  in  1919  decided  to  graft  into 
Cox  Orange,  but  only  the  tops  of  the  trees  were  completed.  The  Cox  Orange 
came  into  bearing  in  a  few  years  and  bore  satisfactory  crops,  but  there  was  no 
benefit  to  the  Blenheims,  which  continued  to  bear  little  or  nothing. 

In  1931,  one  of  these  trees  was  tented  and  a  hive  of  bees  introduced.  The  set 
of  fruit  obtained  was  very  high,  viz.,  13-72  per  cent,  but  the  open  pollinated  trees 
yielded  9  •  83  per  cent,  which  is  also  very  high  and  the  difference  between  the  two 
is  not  particularly  significant.  Any  other  year  the  results  might  have  been  very 
different,  and  the  danger  of  drawing  conclusions  from  one  year's  results  is  thereby 
emphasized. 


78 


An  interesting  result  obtained  in  this  experiment  is  the  low  percentage  fruit 
produced  by  the  Cox  Orange  limbs  inside  the  tent,  as  a  result  of  selfing  or  Blen- 
heim crosses,  as  compared  with  the  much  higher  percentage  fruit  on  open  pol- 
linated limbs,  where  cross-fruitful  pollen  was  evidently  available.  Table  No.  10 
gives  the  results. 

TABLE  No.  10.— TABLE  SHOWING  FRUIT,  ETC.,  ON  TENTED  AND  UNTENTED 
BLENHEIM— COX  ORANGE  TREES 


Location  of  station 

Variety 

Number 
blooming 

spurs 
counted 

Average 
number 
blossoms 

per 

blooming 

spur 

Per  cent 
fruit  to 
bloom 

In  tent 

In  tent 

Blenheim 

Cox  Orange 

502 
501 
720 
516 

4-85 
400 
5-18 
4-29 

13-72 
4-71 

Outside  tent 

Blenheim 

10-97 

Cox  Orange. . . 

9-83 

Note:  These  trees  were  Blenheim  with  top  of  tree  top- worked  to  Cox  Orange. 

(vii)  General  Summary  jor  Variety. — Blenheim  under  most  conditions  is 
highly  self-unfruitful,  but  may  occasionally  give  reasonable  yields  even  when 
selfed.  It  is  very  fruitful  as  a  female  parent  when  pollinated  with  an  effective 
pollinizer,  but  is  a  particularly  poor  male  parent. 

(c)  STARK 

(i)  Results  of  Other  Workers. — Very  little  information  is  forthcoming  as  to 
the  fruiting  habits  of  Stark.  Ballard  (1914)  reports  no  set  when  pollinated  with 
Red  Astrachan  in  Maryland.  As  a  male  parent  it  is  reported  to  have  given  3-5 
per  cent  fruit  on  Northern  Spy,  by  Marshall,  Johnston,  et  al.  (1929)  ;  no  set  is 
reported  as  having  been  obtained  when  used  on  Mcintosh  in  Washington  by 
Morris  (1920)  and  the  same  result  was  obtained  by  Ballard  (1916)  on  Yellow 
Transparent. 


(ii)  Results 

of  Selfing  Tests 

on  Stark  (1928-1932). 

Total    blossoms 

Total  set 

Per  cent  set 

Total  fruit 

Per  cent  fruit 

3,073 

280 

911 

106 

3-45 

Hand  pollination  tests  at  Kentville  indicate  a  degree  of  self-fruitfulness  for 
the  variety,  viz.,  9-11  per  cent  "set"  and  3-45  "fruit".  However,  the  latter 
figure  may  not  be  considered  sufficiently  high  to  produce  a  commercial  crop  for 
this  variety,  and  the  planting  of  solid  blocks  should  be  avoided.  The  open 
pollinated  trees  over  a  three-year  average  (1930-1932),  gave  a  percentage  fruit 
of  7-73,  which  may  be  considered  a  good  commercial  crop  for  the  variety. 

(iii)   Results  of  Stark  as  a  Female  Parent  (1928-1932). 


Total  blossoms 

Total  set 

Per  cent  set 

Total  fruit                 Per  cent  fruit 

27,234 

9,453 

34-71 

2154                             7-91 

Stark,  from  our  results,  may  be  termed  a  good  female  parent,  a  percentage 
"  set  "  of  34-71  and  "  fruit  "  7-91  being  obtained  over  a  five-year  average  (1928- 
1932).   Our  figures  represent  the  massed  results  of  some  thirty-five  male  parents 


79 


Fig.  25. — Photographs  illustrating  results  of  experimental  pollina- 
tions: (1)  Stark  selfed;  (2)  Stark  x  Wagener;  (3)  Stark  x 
Gravenstein   (original). 


80 


on  this  variety,  the  larger  number  of  which  were  selected  because  of  their  high 
pollen  germination  ability,  and  a  fairly  high  percentage  "  fruit "  might  be 
expected.  Its  value  as  a  female  parent  in  these  tests,  however,  is  only  slightly 
higher  than  that  of  the  open  pollinated  trees  and  indicates  that  the  variety  is 
fruitful  when  effectively  pollinated. 


(iv)  Results  of  Stark  as 

a  Male  Parent  (1928-1932). 

Total  blossoms 

Total  set 

Per  cent  set 

Total  fruit 

Per  cent  fruit 

Per  cent  seeds 

1,900 

84 

4-42 

49 

2-58 

14-73 

As  a  male  parent,  Stark  has  given  inferior  results,  being  similar  to  Blenheim 
in  this  respect.  The  tests  over  the  five-vear  period  show  a  percentage  "  set  "  of 
4-42  and  "fruit  "2-58. 


(v)  Summary  of  Results  of  All  Varieties  on  Stark  (1928-1932). 


Male  Parent 

Total 
blossoms 

Per  cent 

set 

Per  cent 
fruit 

Baldwin 

603 
410 
537 

2,808 
86 

2,451 
630 
672 
638 
530 
414 

2,164 

2,062 
663 
241 
791 

2.547 
670 
199 
407 
213 
523 
337 
568 
154 
229 
488 

2,221 
717 
255 
383 
610 
536 
477 

23-38 
59-76 
60-71 

9-22 
10-47 
33-66 
42-86 
70-39 
36-36 
20  00 
53-38 
36-88 
14-35 
41-78 
52-28 
60-18 
22-26 
67-31 
57-79 
23-34 
6714 
49-33 
28-49 
19-54 
81-82 
55-90 

7-79 
40-84 
37-80 
59-61 
50-91 
59-34 

504 
69-39 

4-64 

Ben  Davis 

13  17 

Bishop  Pippin 

6- 15 

Blenheim 

2-92 

8-14 

Cox  Orange 

9-83 

Crimson  Beauty 

9-52 

7-89 

Duchess 

7-84 

7-36 

Fameuse 

8-94 

Golden  Russet. . .  . 

11-88 

Gravenstein 

5-63 

Grimes  Golden 

513 

Hubbardston 

7  05 

Jonathan 

8-47 

King 

6-60 

Mcintosh 

12-24 

Melba 

4  •  02 

Nonpareil 

7-62 

Ontario 

704 

Red  Astrachan 

10-71 

R.  I.  Greening 

7-41 

Ribston 

8-63 

Rome  Beauty 

714 

Spy 

10-92 

Stayman  Winesap  

2-46 

Wagener 

11-35 

Wealthy. . . 

614 

Wellington 

10-20 

Winter  Banana 

809 

Wolf  River 

15-25 

Yellow  Transparent 

1-87 

York  Imperial 

8-60 

(vi)  General  Summary  for  Variety. — Starks  are  more  self-fruitful  than 
Blenheims,  but  not  to  the  extent  of  yielding  commercial  crops  when  planted  in 
solid  blocks.  They  are  very  fruitful  when  pollinated  with  diploids  and  give  a 
low  percentage  "  fruit  "  when  used  as  male  parents. 


81 

TABLE  No.  11.— THE  FRUITFULNESS  OF  DIFFERENT  TYPES  OF  BLENHEIM  AND 

STARK  CROSSES  (1928-1932) 


Total 
blossoms 

Total 

set 

Per  cent 

set 

Total 
fruit 

Per  cent 
fruit 

Seeds* 

Cross 

Per  cent 

Average 
number 
per  apple 

Bleinheim  x  Diploids 

Blenheim  x  Triploids 

15,141 

8,490 
6,624 
5,279 
11,599 
9,332 
6,303 
3,073 

3,623 

603 

1,562 

124 

5,183 

1,565 

2,705 

280 

23-93 

7-10 

23-58 

2-35 

44-68 

16-77 

42-92 

9-11 

1,689 

276 

608 

49 

1,173 
499 
482 
106 

1116 
3-25 
9-18 
0-93 

1011 
5-35 
7-64 
3-45 

36-47 
9-31 

35  06 
2-66 
37-18 
10-38 
23-56 
6-80 

4-56 
3-41 

Blenheim  x  Others 

4-47 

Blenheim  Selfed. . .  . 

2-73 

Stark  x  Diploids. . . . 

4-23 

Stark  x  Triploids 

Stark  x  Others 

2-76 
3-44 

Stark  Selfed 

2-90 

*  Seed  counts  made  on  basis  of  actual  number  of  fruit  harvested. 

{d)  GENERAL    RESULTS    AND    CONCLUSIONS 

1.  The  general  results  obtained  by  the  various  tests  on  Blenheim  and  Stark 
lead  to  general  conclusions  similar  to  those  obtained  with  standard  varieties. 
The  behaviour  of  the  two  varieties,  both  triploids,  is  very  similar.  Stark  is, 
however,  more  self-fruitful  than  Blenheim.  Both  varieties  are  fruitful  when 
pollinated  by  diploid  varieties  and  both  give  inferior  results  as  male  parents 
on  all  varieties  tested. 

2.  Blenheim  is  ordinarily  one  of  the  most  self-unfruitful  varieties  grown  in 
the  Province,  giving  an  average  of  0-93  per  cent  over  a  five-year  period  when 
self-pollinated.  The  fact  that  self-fruitfulness  is  not  a  fixed  character  is,  how- 
ever, well  exemplified  in  our  studies  of  this  variety,  which  in  1931  gave  a  per- 
centage "  fruit"  of  5-23  when  selfed.  Stark  gives  more  uniform  results  over  this 
period  with  an  average  of  3*45  per  cent  in  selfing  tests. 


d/e/7/?e/mZ.92% 
345% 
Oraye/7s/e/s7  S63% 

|  CoxOras?<?<?  S83j£ 
1  /Yafener //JS/C 
|  G<?/afe/7#t/sse///.88^ 

S/ar/:  2.53'/ 

Grayens/e/s?  3.88£ 
~  3a/a</r/f7  5./S^ 

Warner  8  Od^ 
Sfc/s7/os/z8./6/{ 


Y*)fJr)*^fcsl 


Cox  Orar7$d>/j.69% 


Fig.  26.— Effect  of  various  male  parents  on  Stark  and  Blen- 
heim— per  cent  of  fruit  after  July  drop  (average  for  five 
years,  1928-32)    (original). 

3.  Both  varieties  appear  to  require  greater  provision  for  suitable  cross- 
pollination  than  most  others  and  this  fact  must  be  taken  into  account  in  all 
new  plantings  or  in  grafting-out  operations.  Furthermore,  wherever  these  varie- 
ties occur  in  mixed  plantings  the  fact  that  both  give  very  inferior  results  as 
male  parents,  even  to  the  extent  of  inhibiting  selfing  in  some  cases  (see  results 
of  tent  experiments),  must  also  be  recognized. 

60796—6 


82 

4.  Among  commercial  varieties,  the  early  blossoming  sorts  such  as  Wagener 
and  Golden  Russet  are  excellent  pollen  parents  for  both  varieties.  Cox  Orange 
gives  good  results  in  hand  pollinations,  but,  if  depended  upon  exclusively,  it 
may  be  somewhat  late  in  blossoming  in  some  seasons.  The  value  of  these  male 
parents  is  indicated  in  the  seed  counts,  as  was  found  to  be  the  case  with  the 
standard  varieties. 

5.  Detailed  results  are  presented  in  the  accompanying  table  and  chart. 

8.    PLANNING    THE    ORCHARD 

In  considering  the  question  of  setting  out  new  orchards  or  grafting  our  old 
ones  several  points  require  consideration: 

1.  The  planting  of  blocks  of  self-unfruitful  varieties,  which  includes  all  the 
varieties  listed  by  us  except  Baldwin,  should  be  avoided. 

2.  The  proper  admixture  of  cross-fruitful  varieties,  of  which  the  bloom 
overlaps  sufficiently  to  permit  cross-pollination,  should  be  given  consideration. 

3.  If  the  bloom  does  not  exactly  coincide,  it  is  preferable  for  that  of  the 
variety  introduced  as  a  pollinizer  to  be  a  little  earlier  rather  than  a  little  later 
than  that  of  the  variety  it  is  desired  to  pollinate. 

4.  The  varieties  used  as  pollinizers  must  themselves  be  provided  with  pollin- 
izers,  as  many  varieties  that  produce  excellent  pollen  for  crossing  purposes  are 
themselves  quite  self-unfruitful. 

The  best  supply  of  pollen  is  that  provided  by  effective  pollinizers  planted 
at  suitable  intervals  in  the  orchard.  Authorities  differ  as  to  how  many  are 
required,  some  advising  at  least  every  fourth  tree  in  every  fourth  row,  others 
claiming  that  a  full  row  of  the  pollinizer  every  second  or  third  row  is  not  too 
great.  It  is  difficult  to  give  definite  recommendations  from  the  evidence  avail- 
able, especially  when  the  effect  of  over-pollination  is  considered,  but,  from  a 
study  of  all  the  factors  involved  it  would  appear  that  the  provision  of  a  full 
row  every  fourth  row  would  represent  the  minimum  provision  for  pollination 
where  only  diploid  varieties  such  as  Golden  Russet,  Wagener,  Cox  Orange  or 
Northern  Spy  occur  in  the  block  concerned.  The  evidence  at  hand  indicates  that 
in  certain  seasons  pollination  is  very  local  and  that,  in  order  to  provide  against 
such  conditions,  even  closer  planting  than  that  indicated  would  be  advantageous. 
The  need  for  special  provision  of  abundant  pollinizers  is  most  acute  in  the  case 
of  such  varieties  as  Blenheim  and  Stark,  and  is  of  importance  wherever  triploid 
varieties  form  a  large  proportion  of  the  planting.  Full  rows  seem  to  be  prefer- 
able to  scattered  trees  under  these  conditions.  The  habit  of  bees  of  working  a 
limited  locality  is  an  important  consideration. 

The  blossoming  period  of  Gravenstein  and  King,  for  example,  sufficiently 
overlaps  that  of  Golden  Russet  or  Wagener  to  enable  the  former  to  be  pollinated 
by  the  latter,  but  neither  King  nor  Gravenstein  will  give  besl  results  as  pollin- 
izers for  other  varieties.  Gravenstein,  however,  is  superior  to  King  in  this 
respect.  Golden  Russet  and  Wagener  are  inter-fruitful,  and  air  sufficiently  over- 
lapped by  Cox  Orange  to  give  fairly  satisfactory  results.  Cox  Orange  i-  also  an 
excellent  pollinizer  for  Baldwin,  and  Golden  Russet  also  overlap>  sufficiently  to  be 
of  service,  especially  to  the  earlier  bloom.  Spy  presents  the  greatest  problem  of 
our  commercial  sorts.  Cox  Orange  overlaps  to  a  sufficient  extent  in  most  years 
to  give  a  good  crop,  but  in  seasons  where  the  bloom  is  more  prolonged  Ben 
Davis  gives  better  results.  However.  Ben  Davis  is  no  longer  recommended  for 
new  plantings.  Rome  Beauty  has  given  good  results  elsewhere,  but  there 
appears  to  be  some  difference  of  opinion  as  to  the  commercial  value  of  Rome 
Beauty  in  the  Valley,  owing  mainly  to  the  poor  growth  habits  of  the  tree.  In 
a  season  like  1930  when  the  bloom  came  and  went  practicably  within  a  week. 
there  is  not  the  same  problem  as  regards  overlapping  of  bloom,  but  in  some 
seasons  the  unevenness  of  bloom  of  the  different  varieties  constitutes  a  difficulty. 


83 


MAV 


JUME 


4rk.B/c*ck. 

a 

£   2 

/    g 

8    i 

S    2 

a  i 

N 

Z     I 

5    * 

>    i 

6 

7 

e 

9 

l( 

1 

B 

.     13 

M 

!•    15 

£&/£/#//?            i 

3oskoop         _| 

A 

3a/?ks 

3axfer 

3er?Da^/'s 

3/ack£e/7/?a,y/5 

3/e/?/?e//77 

fioughftvee/ 

▲ 

3/5kopP/pp//7 

| 

Dram/ei/ 

CfrasPoss 

CbxOrarrqe 

1 

CoxPo/nona' 

Cr/msondeai/fy  _ 
Dekc/ovs        _^ 

B— 

Disc/?ess 

7Zr//arivafer       _j 

▲ 

/Yr/neuse 

Oa/70 

^^ 

Go/ate/? Pusse/ 

Gn/rres Go/den  _, 

,     . 

//ubbarafs/o/? 

1 

Uonafkcrr? 

K/nq 

^ 

k 

K/nkeaaf 

Lcrbo 

▲ 

A7a/tfe/?'s 3/usf?  _ 

A 

Afa/r?/r?of/?D7fv/q  _J 

J 

^^ 

Sfc/nfosh 

B     a] 

Sfe/ba 

| 

/*f//iYaukee 

/Yew/ownP/pp/n 

/Yonpare/7 

/YW.Gree/7//?q 

Onfar/o 

Rec/XJs/rachct/7  _+ 

P/bs/on 

P.  /  Green /nq    . 

Pome  Beauty    . 

1 

PtYPe/neke 

_ _ 

Sa/omre 

A 

5eek-k?o-Fur/ber  ! 

Spi/ 

5/ark 

Sfai//r7ar?M/7esap 

St '  lawrence 

Ta/ma/7 

1     , 

Waqener 

-^ 

B-- 

/Yea/fni/ 

L^ 

/Ye///nqfon 

/Y/nter3a>/7ana 

W/r?terR/bs/on 

Wo/fP/rer 

/Vorce5/erFbar/7?a//7_A 

^ 

>e//oiY7ra/75pare/?/ 

^ 

k. 

ybrk /mper/cr/ 

^^ 

Fig.  27. — Chart  showing  average  blooming  period  of  different  varieties 
(1924-31)    (original). 


r96—6i 


84 

However,  Cox  Orange  can  be  depended  upon  to  give  at  least  fair  results  on  Spyr 
and  owing  to  the  fact  that  this  variety  blossoms  very  unevenly,  it  is  suitable  for 
pollinating  a  wider  range  of  varieties  than  almost  any  other.  When  the  pollen 
of  Golden  Russet  or  Wagener  is  dried  and  properly  stored  it  gives  excellent 
results  on  Spy,  but  under  orchard  conditions  the  difference  in  the  blooming  period 
is  too  great  to  make  them  dependable  pollinizers  for  the  latter  variety.  If 
Mcintosh  is  used,  it  works  well  with  the  medium  blooming  varieties,  including 
Golden  Russet. 


A 

o    ■ 

• 

o 

„ 

• 

o 

»   © 

, 

O 

0 

© 

„ 

O 

, 

* 

, 

o 

© 

o 

, 

o 

, 

-    e 

>     •'    © 

'       © 

•      ( 

►      ° 

© 

© 

9 

• 

© 

o 

© 

• 

* 

6 

° 

o 

o 

© 

o    • 

• 

6 

• 

Q 

>   © 

o 

• 

© 

o 

© 

0 

© 

• 

o 

« 

© 

» 

o 

e 

o 

• 

•    e 

4 

>     •     © 

'       © 

•     4 

►      ° 

© 

• 

© 

0 

© 

o' 

© 

o 

© 

• 

© 

o 

o 

o 

o 

© 

o    ■ 

© 

• 

O 

•   © 

• 

o 

o 

© 

0 

o 

• 

© 

0 

© 

o 

© 

o 

o 

« 

o 

•    e 

>     •     © 

© 

•     ( 

>      ° 

9 

0 

© 

© 

o 

© 

© 

o 

- 

• 

o 

« 

o 

■ 

© 

o    ■ 

• 

© 

• 

o 

■   © 

. 

© 

o 

© 

• 

© 

0 

* 

0 

Q 

o 

© 

o 

o 

o 

o 

» 

•    e 

►     •     © 

'       © 

•      i 

9 

• 

© 

o 

© 

o 

9 

o 

© 

0 

• 

o 

• 

o 

» 

0 

o    ■ 

• 

6 

# 

© 

'   © 

• 

© 

o 

© 

o 

G 

o 

* 

• 

O 

o 

© 

o 

o 

» 

o 

• 

•    e 

>     •     © 

© 

i 

> 

9 

• 

© 

• 

© 

° 

* 

• 

9 

• 

e 

• 

o 

• 

o 

• 

© 

0 

©  ■' 

$ 

© 

• 

o 

.   © 

. 

0 

• 

* 

• 

© 

* 

o 

o 

0 

© 

o 

o 

0 

o 

• 

•    © 

>      •     © 

© 

°      ( 

►      • 

© 

• 

e 

© 

o 

* 

© 

» 

a 

• 

0 

o 

o 

o    . 

• 

© 

• 

o 

•    © 

• 

© 

° 

- 

• 

© 

o 

* 

■      o 

o 

o 

9 

> 

6 

• 

6 

• 

■    e 

i 

>      •     © 

© 

°      i 

>      ° 

9 

• 

* 

0 

© 

o 

* 

o 

9 

o 

* 

O 

o 

• 

o 

o 

© 

O     ' 

• 

6 

• 

© 

■   © 

• 

G 

• 

e 

• 

Q 

o 

- 

• 

o 

o 

3 

» 

o 

« 

o 

• 

•    e 

►     .     ©. 

■     © 

o             { 

)      • 

© 

• 

* 

o 

© 

o 

© 

» 

© 

• 

* 

o 

0 

o. 

o 

o 

o 

©   ' 

• 

6 

• 

o 

•   © 

o 

o 

• 

© 

• 

• 

o 

o 

© 

o 

o 

•    e 

>     .     © 

■     © 

)           o 

© 

• 

e 

■ 

© 

o 

* 

© 

• 

* 

o 

0 

•6 

.© 

©    ' 

• 

© 

• 

© 

-   © 

• 

0 

• 

© 

© 

• 

• 

• 

o 

o 

© 

a 

o 

o 

-  e 

•     4 

>     •     © 

■     © 

•     i 

>     ° 

© 

• 

- 

* 

9 

• 

© 

* 

© 

' 

© 

• 

o 

• 

6 

6 

3  3a/d*/sj 

©  Go/o'er? Xussef 

JOO  Trees 

.  -Hya/vr/ 

\  Grave/ts/e//? 

© 

A/cap 

300  r///ers 

( 

3  CoxOrawfe 

• 

Mkre&rer '.(fyOgr) 

9.8  acres 

Fig.   28. — Suggested   plan   for   a   five-variety   orchard   with   fillers    (original). 

For  Blenheim,  Golden  Russet  and  Wagener  provide  suitable  pollinizers  and 
Mcintosh  works  fairly  well.  Cox  Orange,  in  some  seasons,  is  late  for  best 
results.  The  same  varieties  are  satisfactory  for  Stark.  In  providing  pollinizers 
for  Blenheim  and  Stark  it  would  seem  to  be  advisable  to  do  so  in  greater 
abundance  than  for  the  standard  varieties,  in  order  to  make  pollination  more 
certain.  It  is  suggested,  in  setting  out  poor  pollen-producing  varieties,  that  not 
more  than  two  rows  be  placed  together,  followed  by  a  row  or  two  of  a  pollinizing 
variety,  which,  in  turn,  should  be  followed  by  one  or  two  rows  of  a  variety 
cross-fruitful  with  the  latter.     Whether  one  or  two  rows  would  be  used  would 


o  o  • 

0 

0 

• 

o 

o 

•  o  o 

o  o  e 

o 

o 

e 

o 

o 

e  o  o 

o  o  • 

o 

o 

• 

o 

o 

•  o  o 

o  o  e 

o 

o 

e 

o 

o 

e  o  o 

o  o  • 

o 

o 

• 

o 

o 

•  o  o 

o  o  e 

o 

o 

e 

o 

o 

e  o  o 

o  o  • 

o 

o 

• 

o 

o 

•  o  o 

o  o  e 

o 

o 

e 

o 

0 

e  o  o 

o  o  • 

o 

o 

• 

o 

o 

•  o  o 

o  o  e 

o 

o 

e 

o 

o 

e  o  o 

Q)B/enhe/m 

QCoxOranqe 

#  Go/de/7  Russer 

Flg.  29. — Possible  plan  for  top-working 
solid  block  of  Blenheim  to  give  mini- 
mum number  of  pollinizers.  Golden 
Russet  used  to  pollinate  Blenheim 
and  Cox  Orange  to  pollinate  Golden 
Russet    (original). 


85 

depend  upon  the  proportion  of  each  that  it  is  desired  to  have  in  the  orchard. 
Where  it  is  necessary  to  top-work  a  block  of  a  self-unfruitful  variety,  such  as 
Blenheim,  for  the  purpose  of  improving  the  set,  every  third  row  should  be  worked 
over,  one  half  with  a  variety  introduced  primarily  to  pollinate  Blenheim  and 
one  half  with  a  variety  introduced  primarily  to  pollinate  the  pollinizer  of  the 
Blenheim.  In  introducing  varieties  for  pollinizing  purposes,  the  most  desirable 
commercial  sorts  should  be  selected. 


0O©© 

e  e  •  • 

•    • 

G 

e  ©  ©  0  0 

00©© 

e  e  •  • 

•     • 

e 

©  ©  ©  0  0 

00©© 

e  e  #  # 

•    • 

e 

©  ©  ©  0  0 

0    0©© 

e  e  •  • 

•    • 

e 

©©©00 

0    0   ©   © 

e  e  •  • 

•    • 

© 

©©©00 

O0©© 

e  e  •  • 

•    • 

e 

©©©00 

O    0    ©   © 

e  ©  •  • 

•    • 

e 

©  ©  ©  0  0 

O0©© 

e  e  •  • 

•    • 

e 

©©©00 

OO©© 

e  e  •  • 

•     • 

e 

©©©00 

0   0©© 

e  e  •  • 

•    • 

e 

©©©00 

O  Cox  Orange 

©  /fcfo/os/r    ©  Oo/aer?  Russet  %  6nrve/?s/e/s7 

Fig.  30. — Suggested  plan   for   a   four-variety  orchard    (orginal). 

The  accompanying  charts  showing  (1)  average  blossoming  periods  of  varie- 
ties (2)  the  cross-fruitfulness  of  varieties  and  (3)  typical  combinations  of  varie- 
ties, will  illustrate  the  application  of  the  results  of  the  foregoing  investigations.. 


(jrarensfe. 


/V&<?/?er 


A'//?4p 


ga/afr/, 


&>/Oe>/? 


Cbx  Orer/r^e- 


&>? 


Fig.  31.— Chart  showing  inter-fruitfulness  of  some  standard1 
varieties.   Arrows  indicate  direction  of  cross   (original). 

Ben  Davis  is  added,  though  not  a  recommended  variety,  because  of  abundance 
in  commercial  planting  and  superior  results  on  Northern  Spy.. 


86 


9.  RELATION   OF  FRUIT   SET   ON   ENTIRE  TREE  TO   THAT   ON 
INDIVIDUAL    LIMBS    OR    SPURS 

It  has  already  been  indicated  elsewhere  that  where  the  whole  tree  is  exposed 
to  effective  cross-pollination,  individually  pollinated  limbs  give  lower  yields  than 
when  the  remainder  of  the  tree  is  ineffectively  pollinated  or  not  pollinated  at  all. 

This  is  most  clearly  brought  out  in  tented  trees  of  such  self-unfruitful 
varieties  as  Gravenstein  and  Spy.  The  figures  in  the  accompanying  table  show 
that,  as  a  rule,  the  lower  the  set  on  the  tree,  the  higher  the  set  on  the  individual 
limb  pollinated,  and  vice  versa,  though  not,  of  course,  in  direct  proportion. 

TABLE  No.  12.— EFFECT  OF  NUTRITIONAL  COMPETITION  ON  TENTED  TREES  (1928-32) 


Variety 


Treatment 


Whole  trees 


Per  cent  Per  cent 
set  fruit 


Limb  hand 

pollinated 

with  an 

effective 

pollinizer 


Per  cent  Per  cent 
set  fruit 


Gravenstein 

King 

Baldwin. . .  . 
Spy 


No  bees  and  no  bouquets 

Bees  and  effective  bouquets 

No  bees  and  no  bouquets 

Bees  and  effective  bouquets 
No  bees  and  no  bouquets. . . . 
Bees  and  effective  bouquets 
No  bees  and  no  bouquets. . . 
Bees  and  effective  bouquets 


0-90 
21-18 

413 
30  11 

8-23 
3111 

1-09 
49-21 


0-67 
10-90 
103 
5-42 
3-49 
8-17 
0-85 
10  05 


70-66 
34-80 
37-63 
29-28 
43  05 
45-41 
66-90 
83-09 


4711 
18-38 
11-87 
7-32 
19-15 
12-69 
26-34 
18-18 


10.   OVER-POLLINATION 

MacDaniels  (1931)  has  emphasized  the  factor  of  over-pollination  in  con- 
nection with  biennial  bearing.  It  was  noted  in  our  tented  series,  especially  in  the 
Gravenstein  variety,  that  trees  supplied  with  bees  and  an  effective  pollinizer  and 
yielding  heavy  crops,  failed  to  give  a  crop  the  following  year.  On  the  other  hand 
where  blossoming  trees  were  prevented  from  bearing  through  lack  of  insect  pol- 
linators, there  was  a  strong  tendency  to  bear  abnormally  large  crops  the  follow- 
ing year.  In  other  words,  what  amounted  to  defloration  took  place.  Where  bees 
and  ineffective  bouquets  were  used  in  tented  series,  and  only  a  small  crop  resulted, 
the  trees  in  the  following  years  tended  to  approach  an  annual  habit.  The  above 
data  tend  to  indicate  that  the  amount  of  pollination  that  occurs  in  any  given 
year  has  an  influence  on  succeeding  crops. 

Baldwin  is  the  one  variety  under  test  that  is  most  outstanding  in  its  bien- 
nial bearing  habit  in  the  Valley  orchards,  a  high  percentage  of  spurs  blossoming 
each  year.  It  is  also  our  most  self-fruitful  variety.  It  is  a  well  known  fact  that, 
when  Baldwin  blossoms  it,  unlike  some  other  varietur,  almost  invariably  fruits. 
Pollination  of  this  variety  is  seldom  lacking  owing  to  its  self-fruitful  habits. 
With  little  cross-pollination,  it  tends  to  bear  heavy  crops,  so  as  usually  to  require 
thinning.  This  is  ordinarily  followed  by  a  pronounced  "  off  year  ". 

At  the  opposite  extreme  is  the  King  variety,  which,  in  our  tests,  rarely  gave  a 
large  percentage  of  fruit  from  a  given  number  of  blossoms.  Even  where  the 
original  set  was  heavy,  abscission  was  correspondingly  heavy,  so  that  there  was 
less  difference  than  is  ordinarily  the  case  between  selfing,  good  pollinizers  and 
poor  pollinizers.  Coincident  with  this  we  have  the  annual  bearing  habit  more 
pronounced  in  this  variety  in  the  average  Valley  orchard. 


87 

11.  TEMPORARY  PROVISION  OF  POLLEN 

(a)  POLLINIZING  BOUQUETS 

As  a  temporary  means  of  supplying  suitable  pollen,  before  grafting  operations 
can  become  effective,  the  use  of  bouquets  has  been  advocated.  These  consist  of 
blossoming  limbs  of  the  desired  varieties  placed  in  tubs  of  water  in  the  orchard 
or  hung  in  the  branches.  Results  from  such  methods  are  apt  to  be  disappointing, 
as  indicated  elsewhere.  It  is  difficult  to  furnish  a  sufficient  mass  of  bloom  to  be 
effective  over  the  much  greater  proportion  of  ineffective  pollen  present.  It  also  is 
difficult  to  keep  such  bloom  fresh  or  in  good  condition,  especially  in  hot  dry 
weather.  Bouquets  are  ordinarily  not  as  freely  visited  by  bees  as  the  greater 
masses  of  bloom  provided  by  whole  trees  and,  if  allowed  to  wilt,  may  become 
quite  unattractive.  Furthermore,  it  is  a  method  that  is  not  generally  popular 
with  growers. 

(b)  HAND  POLLINATION 

As  an  alternative  to  the  temporary  use  of  bouquets,  MacDaniels  (1930)  has 
suggested  the  hand  pollination  of  the  orchard.  He  claims  that,  hand  pollination 
of  apples  when  the  weather  is  unfavourable  for  natural  cross-pollination  gives 
very  successful  results,  the  blossoms  to  be  picked  just  before  opening,  the  un- 
opened anthers  pulled  off  and  spread  on  paper  trays  in  a  warm  room  to  dry ;  the 
pollen  should  then  be  placed  in  small  unstoppered  bottles  and  applied  to  the  trees 
with  a  camel's  hair  brush  as  soon  as  possible.  On  a  heavy  blossoming  tree  he 
states  that  it  would  only  be  necessary  to  pollinate  20  to  25  per  cent  of  the  blos- 
soming spurs  to  get  a  full  crop,  nor  should  all  the  flowers  in  a  cluster  be  treated. 
With  experience  it  is  said  that  a  tree  15  years  old  capable  of  bearing  10-15  bushels 
can  be  pollinated  in  an  hour  and  a  half.  Another  method  described  is  to  cut 
branches  of  a  good  pollinizing  variety  when  pollen  is  being  shed  and  to  brush  the 
mother  trees  with  these. 

(c)  THE  "  BEE  POLLEN-COATER  " 

Yet  another  method  of  furnishing  a  temporary  supply  of  pollen  has  been 
experimented  with  by  Burrell  and  King  (1931)  and  consists  of  a  device  known  as 
a  "  pollen-coater  ".  Its  purpose  is  to  force  the  bees  to  walk  through  a  quantity 
of  pollen  on  entering  or  leaving  the  hive. 

The  bee  pollen-coater  is  a  modification  of  what  is  known  to  beekeepers  as 
a  winter  hive  entrance  block.  The  latter  is  essentially  a  diagonal  horizontal 
tunnel  about  6  inches  long,  3  inches  wide  and  ^-inch  high,  attached  to  the  front 
of  the  hive.  Its  ordinary  use  is  to  permit  free  passage  of  the  bees  in  and  out 
of  the  hive  at  will,  but  at  the  same  time,  to  check  air  currents  that  would  chill 
the  colony. 

For  the  present  purpose  the  roof  of  the  tunnel  is  removed.  Two  wooden 
strips  each  ^-inch  high  are  nailed  across  the  floor  of  the  tunnel,  thus  forming  an 
enclosure  to  contain  the  pollen.  A  glass  plate  is  substituted  for  the  original 
roof  of  the  tunnel  so  that  one  may  observe  the  bees  and  determine  readily  when 
the  pollen  supply  needs  replenishment.  A  piece  of  wood  is  laid  on  the  glass 
plate  in  sunny  weather  to  prevent  excessive  heating  of  the  pollen.  The  workers 
referred  to  above  consider  that  this  device  holds  promise  of  successfully  solving 
the  problem  of  pollen  distribution  in  the  absence  of  suitable  pollinizers  in  the 
orchard. 

(d)  USE  OF  ORCHARD  DUSTER  WITH  BOUQUETS 

Still  another  method  has  been  tried  by  us  with  good  results  during  the 
season  of  1932,  and  it  has  much  to  commend  it  over  either  hand  pollination  or 
the  use  of  bouquets.  It  consisted  in  placing  a  bouquet  of  a  cross-fruitful  variety 
adjacent  to  the  tree  to  be  pollinated,  and  blowing  through  it  a  current  of  air 
from  an  ordinary  orchard  duster.    The  work  must  be  done  very  thoroughly  and 


88 

the  draft  directed  through  the  bouquet  in  all  directions.  Provided  the  anthers 
are  in  the  proper  condition  the  pollen  may  be  blown  right  out  of  the  anthers  and 
results  obtained  similar  to  those  secured  in  hand  pollinations.  The  results  as 
set  forth  in  the  accompanying  table  may  be  compared  with  those  from  the  tented 
series  or  with  those  secured  through  hand  pollinations.  The  air  velocity 
obtained  at  different  distances  from  the  outlet  is  given  in  another  table. 

TABLE  No.  13.— RESULTS  OF  TESTS  IN  FORCED  DRAFT  POLLINATION,  1932 


Variety 


Gravenstein 

King 

Baldwin. . . . 
Spy 


Total 

Total 

Per  cent 

Total 

blossoms 

set 

set 

fruit 

2,540 

95 

3-74 

86 

1,841 

399 

21-67 

132 

2,411 

472 

19-58 

242 

2,792 

340 

12  18 

198 

Per  cent 
fruit 


3-39 

7-17 

10  04 

7-09 


The  foregoing  results  indicate  clearly  that  effective  pollination  can  be 
effected  by  this  method.  In  the  Gravenstein  tent  Wagener  bouquets  were  used, 
and  the  increase  over  that  of  the  selfed  tree  was  three  per  cent.  Wagener 
bouquets  were  again  introduced  into  the  King  tent  and  here  we  find  an  increase 
of  four  per  cent  over  that  of  selfing,  which  is  very  significant,  especially  in  the 
case  of  such  a  variety  as  King.  Cox  Orange  was  used  as  bouquets  for  the 
Baldwin  tree  and  Ben  Davis  for  the  Spy,  and  again  a  gain  of  three  and  two  per 
cent  respectively,  over  that  of  corresponding  selfed  trees  was  obtained.  Its  use 
from  a  practical  standpoint  is  questionable,  but  it  certainly  offers  a  means  by 
which  a  fruit  grower  can  increase  yields,  where  blocks  of  self-  or  cross-unfruit- 
ful varieties  occur  and  it  requires  less  detailed  work  than  hand  pollination. 

It  may  be  noted  that  the  open  pollinated  trees  in  the  same  set  of  tests, 
proved  only  slightly  better  in  the  case  of  each  variety. 

Table  13  (a)  showing  the  velocity  of  the  wind  inside  and  outside  the 
tent  is  given  for  purposes  of  record  and  because  of  its  bearing  on  wind  pollina- 
tion on  tented  trees.  Considerable  irregularity  is  noted,  but  the  difference 
between  tented  and  untented  trees  is  very  apparent. 

The  measured  air  velocity  at  different  distances  from  the  mouth  of  the  pipe 
is  given  in  table  14. 


89 


TABLE  No.  13  (a)— COMPARISON  OF  WIND  VELOCITY  INSIDE  AND  OUTSIDE    TENT, 

WOLFVILLE,  N.S. 


Wind  in  feet 

Wind  in  feet 

per  minute 

per  minute 

Date 

Time 

Date 

Time 

In 

Outside 

In 

Outside 

tent 

tent 

tent 

tent 

Sept.  2 

2-30 

4-6 

179-0 

Sept.  7 

2-45 

22-8 

3170 

2-35 

1-6 

42-3 

2-50 

2-6 

497-0 

2-40 

2-4 

253-5 

2-55 

11-2 

950 

3-30 

0 

740 

315 

140 

264-0 

3-35 

0 

15-8 

3-20 

3-6 

1160 

3-40 

0 

42-3 

3-25 

0-8 

179-0 

Sept.  5 

4-45 

0 

137-2 

3-45 

0-4 

232-0 

4-50 

0 

950 

3-50 

10-2 

190-0 

4-55 

0 

102-0 

3-55 

18-8 

273-0 

5-00 

0 

1270 

415 

2-2 

157  0 

505 

0 

1270 

4-20 

0-4 

1570 

510 

0 

1790 

4-25 

1-2 

52-8 

Sept.  6 

9-30 

2-8 

338-0 

4-45 

1-4 

52-8 

9-35 

9-6 

3170 

4-50 

0-6 

126-8 

9-40 

0 

158-5 

4-55 

0-6 

105-5 

9-45 

33-6 

1900 

Sept.  9 

200 

107-6 

410-2 

9-50 

57-4 

433  0 

205 

800 

422-0 

9-55 

28-6 

102-0 

210 

142-4 

455-0 

1015 

10-8 

179-0 

3  00 

101-8 

380-5 

10-20 

10 

214-3 

3  05 

97-4 

296-0 

10-25 

2-4 

296-0 

310 

800 

322-0 

2-50 

12-4 

1790 

3-30 

170-5 

518-0 

2-55 

250 

243-0 

3-35 

142-0 

423-0 

300 

170 

296-0 

3-40 

184-1 

465-0 

3-35 

490 

3170 

400 

211-6 

475-0 

3-40 

47-4 

357-0 

4  05 

2150 

581-0 

3-45 

43-2 

348-0 

4-10 

201-8 

5180 

4-35 

19-4 

264-0 

4-30 

189-0 

496  0 

4-40 

1-8 

116-0 

4-35 

163-6 

538-0 

4-45 

24-2 

158-0 

4-40 

126-0 

581-0 

TABLE  No.   14.— VELOCITY  OF  CURRENT   FROM   ORCHARD   DUSTER  IN   RELATION 
TO  DISTANCE  FROM  MOUTH  OF  PIPE 


Distance  from  mouth  of  pipe 

M.P.H. 

Distance  from  mouth  of  pipe 

M.P.H. 

i' 

77-86 
63-12 
48-30 
36-42 
26-55 
21-41 
18-41 
15-74 
13-68 

9 

11-98 

1 

10 

10-82 

2 

11 

9-85 

3                                                      

12 

904 

4.   .                  

13 

8-39 

5                                                

14 

7-81 

6 

15 

7-36 

7 

16 

6-98 

8 

12.  INHIBITING  EFFECT  OF  UNFRUITFUL  POLLEN 

In  some  cases,  though  not  in  all,  there  is  an  indication  that  pollen  from  a 
cross-unfruitful  variety  actually  inhibits  selfing.  This  may  be  true  even  in 
varieties  that  normally  give  very  little  fruit  when  selfed,  but  more  particularly 
in  a  comparatively  self-fruitful  variety  such  as  Baldwin. 

Several  examples  of  the  foregoing  may  be  cited.  On  Gravenstein  tented: 
trees  supplied  with  bees  and  bouquets  of  an  ineffective  pollinizer,  viz.,  Blenheim, 
over  a  four-year  period  gave  an  average  percentage  "  fruit "  of  1  •  14,  and  the 
tented  selfed  Gravenstein  trees  (bees  and  no  bouquets),  gave  2-12  per  cent 
"  fruit "  over  the  same  period,  in  other  words  an  inhibiting  effect  of  nearly  1 
per  cent.  Baldwin,  tented  trees  supplied  with  bees  and  ineffective  bouquets 
(Nonpareil)  yielded  an  average  percentage  "  fruit"  of  4-96.    The  same  variety 


90 

when  selfed  (i.e.  bees  and  no  bouquets)  produced  7-77  per  cent  "  fruit,"  i.e.  sl 
decrease  of  nearly  three  per  cent  fruit,  which  means  the  difference  between  a 
good  and  a  fair  commercial  crop. 

13.    VARIATION    IN    SELF-FRUITFULNESS 

(a)  SEASONAL 

Self-fruitfulness  in  the  apple  appears  to  be  influenced  by  the  climatic  con- 
ditions especially  during  its  period  of  stigma  receptivity.  This  fact  is  well  estab- 
lished in  our  selfing  records  throughout  the  period  of  our  studies.  One  striking- 
example  of  this  variation  became  evident  in  1932.  Spy  in  the  tented  series 
showed  an  average  percentage  "fruit"  of  -93  on  selfed  trees  (i.e.  bees  and  no 
bouquets)  during  the  1929-1931  period,  which  strongly  indicates  self-unfruit- 
fulness.  However,  in  the  season  of  1932  the  tented  selfed  tree  gave  a  percentage 
"  fruit"  of  4-84,  or  in  other  words,  a  commercial  crop  was  produced  by  selfing. 
In  1932,  the  selfed  limbs  in  the  hand  pollinated  tests  on  Gravenstein  showed  a 
percentage  "  fruit  "  of  2-79,  this  figure  being  1-64  per  cent  higher  than  the  five- 
year  average.  On  the  other  hand  selfing  tests  on  Baldwin  in  1932  gave  a  per- 
centage "  fruit  "  of  3-01,  which  was  3-36  per  cent  less  than  the  five-year  average 
and  a  low  percentage  fruit  for  such  a  comparatively  self-fruitful  variety.  In 
1930,  the  latter  variety  showed  a  percentage  "  fruit  "  of  9*50  on  the  hand  selfed 
limbs. 

Hand  pollination  tests  in  1932  indicate  that  King  is  very  self-fruitful,  a 
percentage  "fruit"  of  6-11  being  obtained.  The  average  for  the  five-year 
period  was  3-56.  All  data  cited  indicate  clearly  that  there  is  a  wide  fluctuation 
in  self-fruitfulness  from  year  to  year,  and  a  true  value  for  any  one  variety  can 
only  be  obtained  over  a  period  of  years.  These  differences  are  usually  attributed 
to  "  climate,"  though  it  is  not  always  possible  to  state  what  particular  item  or 
items  in  the  complex  are  responsible. 

(b)  DUE  TO  TECHNIQUE 

Self-fruitfulness  in  apples  varies  in  relation  to  the  experimental  technique 
used  as  is  clearly  portrayed  in  the  accompanying  table.  The  bagged  series 
includes  the  limbs  self-pollinated  by  hand  under  cheesecloth  bags.  The  tented 
series  is  made  up,  first,  of  those  limbs  self-pollinated  by  hand  under  cheesecloth 
bags  within  the  tent  and,  second,  of  the  whole  tree,  exclusive  of  the  individual 
limb,  selfed  by  bees  and  enclosed  in  a  cheesecloth  tent.  All  varieties  when  self- 
pollinated  by  bees  under  tents,  showed  a  higher  degree  of  self-fruitfulness  than 
where  selfed  by  hand  on  the  same  tree.  The  hand  self-pollinated  limbs  under 
cheesecloth  bags  in  the  open,  gave  uniformly  higher  percentages  than  those  hand 
selfed  within  the  tents.  On  the  other  hand,  selfed  limbs  under  cheesecloth  bags 
in  the  open  gave,  in  every  variety  except  King,  slightly  lower  percentages  than 
those  selfed  by  bees  in  the  tents.  In  the  case  of  King  the  percentages  were 
practically  the  same. 

TABLE  NO.  15 


Variety 


Bagged 

series 

1928-1932 


Selfed 
by  hand 


Tented  series, 
1929-1932 


Selfed 
by  hand 


Selfed 
l>v  bees 


Baldwin. .  . . 
Gravenstein 

King 

Spy 


6-37 
115 
3-56 
1  33 


2-22 

0-50 
2-55 
0-73 


3-32 
2  00 


The  necessity  of  taking  into  account  the  technique  used  by  various  workers 
in  considering  their  results,  is  very  plainly  indicated  by  the  foregoing. 


IV.  FIELD  STUDIES  IN  THE  ROLE  OF  INSECTS  IN 
APPLE  POLLINATION 

W.  H,  BRITTAIN 
A.  INTRODUCTION 

The  following  studies  were  conducted  as  a  part  of  the  general  investigation 
of  apple  pollination  in  the  Annapolis  valley,  Nova  Scotia.  In  view  of  the  efforts 
that  have  been  and  are  being  expended  by  various  extension  organizations  with 
a  view  to  increasing  the  use  of  hive  bees  as  orchard  pollinators,  and  in  view  of 
the  absence  of  any  adequate  supply  of  hive  bees  in  the  territory  concerned,  it 
seemed  advisable  to  make  a  careful  study,  not  only  of  the  role  of  hive  bees  in 
orchard  pollination,  but  also  of  the  native  insect  fauna  to  which  most  of  the 
pollination  that  actually  takes  place  must  be  due.  In  this  connection,  it  was 
thought  best  to  conduct  our  studies  from  the  comparative  standpoint,  using  the 
hive  bee  as  the  standard  of  comparison,  since  the  behaviour  of  this  species  has 
been  much  more  fully  studied.  The  main  object  of  these  studies  was  to  determine 
the  abundance  and  distribution  of  the  wild  bee  fauna,  their  relative  value  as 
pollinators  as  compared  with  hive  bees,  the  necessity  or  otherwise  of  supple- 
menting their  activities  by  introducing  hive  bees  and  the  problems  connected 
therewith.  Though  considerable  information  has  been  accumulated,  much  still 
remains  to  be  accomplished  before  all  the  points  dealt  with  are  finally  elucidated. 

Throughout  the  course  of  these  studies  the  writer  has  received  continuous 
assistance  from  Mr.  J.  M.  Cameron,  not  only  in  carrying  out  the  experimental 
work  in  the  field,  but  in  tabulating  and  analyzing  the  data  secured.  Mr.  C.  B. 
Gooderham,  who  appears  as  joint  author  of  one  section  of  this  report,  has  con- 
tributed invaluable  assistance  in  very  many  ways.  Mr.  C.  E.  Atwood  has  taken 
part  in  the  field  studies  at  various  times.  Mr.  John  Leefe  also  performed  useful 
service  during  the  season  of  1932. 

B.  GENERAL 

The  apple  depends  almost  entirely  upon  insects  for  pollination.  While  our 
experiments  show  that  a  certain  amount  of  pollen  may  be  carried  by  wind,  our 
results  are  in  agreement  with  other  workers,  who  find  that  wind  pollination  is 
negligible  in  apple  pollination.  The  fact  must  not  be  lost  sight  of  that  a  suit- 
able pollen  supply  is  of  equal  importance  to  the  work  of  bees  in  order  to  insure 
proper  pollination,  since,  without  effective  pollinizers,  the  activities  of  bees  would 
result  only  in  selfing,  or  in  unfruitful  crosses.  This  fact  is  emphasized  because 
it  has  been  the  practice  of  a  number  of  growers,  when  confronted  with  a  pollina- 
tion problem,  to  endeavour  to  remedy  the  situation  by  introducing  bees  into  the 
orchard,  disregarding  the  primary  necessity  of  ensuring  a  proper  pollen  supply. 

C.   INSECTS   CONCERNED 
1.   HISTORICAL 

Many  workers  have  studied  the  role  of  insects  in  the  pollination  of  apples 
and  a  great  deal  of  valuable  information  is  available  as  a  result  of  these  studies. 
Some  of  the  more  recent  studies  are  worthy  of  consideration  at  this  point.  Among 
those  who  have  paid  attention  to  the  insect  visitors  of  apple  bloom,  the  observa- 
tions of  Britton  and  Viereck  (1906)   are  of  interest. 

91 


92 

These  authors  quote  several  writers  who  have  found  that  honey  bees  are  the 
most  important  insects  engaged  in  pollinating  fruit  flowers,  but  their  own  find- 
ings are  to  the  contrary,  as  honey  bees  were  exceedingly  scarce  in  comparison 
with  other  insects.  Their  observations  were  made  at  New  Haven  and  Branford,. 
where  bee  hives  were  less  than  two  miles  away,  and  wild  honey  bees  present.. 
Two  hundred  and  twenty-nine  insects  of  52  species  were  collected  from  the  apple.. 
These  included  9  species  of  Halictus;  5  species  of  Andrena)  3  of  Trachandrena'y. 
4  of  Bombus;  and  Apis  mellifica;  besides  other  bees  and  wasps — in  all,  32  species, 
of  Hymenoptera.  Osmia  lignaria  was  the  commonest  single  species,  34  individuals 
being  collected  out  of  a  total  of  197  Hymenoptera;  65  Halicitus,  31  Apis,  and  28. 
Andrena  were  also  taken.  Apis  was  comparatively  more  abundant  on  apple- 
than  on  other  fruit  trees.  The  authors  believe  that  most  Diptera  are  of  no. 
importance  as  pollinators,  although  a  few  may  be  considered  beneficial.  They 
also  conclude  that  on  account  of  their  great  numbers,  the  small  bees  belonging 
to  the  Halictidae  and  Andrenidae  were  of  far  greater  importance  in  pollinating 
the  flowers  of  the  plants  from  which  they  were  taken  than  were  the  honey  bees„ 
during  the  seasons  of  1905  and  1906. 

Rawes  and  Wilson  (1922)  have  also  studied  this  question  and  agree  with 
other  workers  that  wind  plays  no  part  in  the  pollination  of  apple  trees.  Insects- 
are  the  only  efficient  pollinators,  and  although  honey  bees  are  active  agents  in 
carrying  pollen,  this  work  may  be  most  efficiently  performed  by  other  insects. 
Among  insects  other  than  hive  bees,  bumble  bees  take  the  foremost  place,  and! 
are  not  kept  from  their  work  by  dark  weather.  Andrena  and  allied  species  are- 
next.  Eristalis,  Syrphus  and  other  small  flies  are  considered  to  play  a  consider- 
able part  in  apple  pollination.  In  regard  to  apples  and  pears,  hive  bees  and  flies 
are  more  frequent  visitors  than  wild  bees  or  other  insects. 

Observations  were  made  by  Hooper  (1929  and  1931)  over  several  years  on 
the  numbers  of  various  insects  visiting  apple  blossoms,  and  the  numbers  added 
up.  The  district  contained  many  cherry,  apple  and  other  fruit  plantations  and 
numbers  of  hive  bees  were  kept.  The  land  not  in  orchard  was  either  ploughed 
land  or  sheep  pasture,  not  very  suitable  for  pollinating  insects.  The  counts  on. 
apple  were  as  follows: 

Hive    bees    374                 Beetles     104 

Bumble   bees    37                  Ants    51 

Halicti,  etc 21                 Earwigs     3 

Flies     23                 Thrips    2 

Fox- Wilson  (1929)  has  also  made  a  careful  study  of  this  question.  This: 
author  gives  a  description  of  Wisley  gardens,  which  are  near  rough  land  and 
pasture.  Various  factors  besides  pollination  which  affect  fruit  setting  are  dis- 
cussed. Most  common  fruit  flowers  are  shown  to  be  entomophilous.  In  order 
to  determine  the  cause  of  attraction  of  insects  to  flowers,  some  apple  blossoms 
had  the  petals  removed.  Hive  bees,  Syrphids  and  Anthomyids,  visited  these 
blossoms.  Bumble  bees  ignored  them.  Artificial  blossoms  without  nectar 
attracted  bumble  bees,  but  hive  bees  ignored  them,  until  nectar  was  placed  in 
them.     Sight  is  considered  more  highly  developed  in  Bombus,  smell  in  Apis. 

The  various  types  of  pollinating  insects  were  discussed  briefly.  Honey 
bees  were  considered  to  be  the  most  efficient  because  of  their  industrious  habits, 
etc.  Bumble  bees  were  less  deterred  by  unfavourable  weather  than  hive  beesv 
and  under  the  conditions  occurring  in  1920,  various  wild  insects  secured  a  good 
crop  with  no  hive  bees  present.  Andrenidae  were  found  very  subject  to  changes 
in  weather.  They  showed  greatest  activity  from  11  to  1  p.m.  and  from  2  to 
4  p.m.,  resting  from  1  to  2  p.m.  Various  other  Hymenoptera  and  Diptera  were 
mentioned  as  visitors  to  fruit  blossoms.  Gnats  of  the  family  Mycetophilidae 
were  found  to  surpass  all  other  insect  visitors  to  apple,  etc.;  but  owing  to  their 
small  size,  are  usually  overlooked.    They  carry  considerable  pollen.    Coleoptera. 


93 

were  found  of  practically  no  use  as  pollinators,  although  frequent  visitors.  The 
number  of  visitors  to  apple  during  a  total  period  of  40  hours,  45  minutes,  com- 
prising observations  in  1920,  1921,  1922,  1923  and  1924  is  given  as  follows:— 

Hive    bees    222  Other    Kvmenoptera    44 

Bumble   bees    337  Diptera     488 

Wild    bees    106  Miscellaneous     132 

"The  author  does  not  state  on  what  quantities  of  bloom  his  observations  were 
:made. 

Hutson  (1926)  quotes  the  work  of  various  investigators  in  regard  to  insects 
other  than  the  honey  bees  concerned  in  pollination  of  fruits.  Calculations  of 
insects  visiting  bloom  between  12  noon  and  1  p.m.  for  three  years  are  given  in 
this  paper.  These  include  Chironomidae,  Muscidae,  Bombidae,  Syrphidae  and 
Scarabaeidae.  Honey  bees  also  were  found  to  work  differently  in  different 
varieties.  There  was  also  a  variation  from  year  to  year,  e.g.,  9  per  minute  in 
1923  and  6  in  1924,  a  variation  of  from  5  to  90  seconds  having  been  observed 
in  length  of  time  spent  on  apple  bloom  when  collecting  nectar.  The  main  point 
brought  out  by  the  collections  was  the  small  number  of  insects  found  in  apples 
during  bloom.  There  was  a  marked  difference  in  the  numbers  found  in  the 
one  orchard  surrounded  by  tilled  land  and  the  planting  surrounded  by  over- 
grown land,  especially  in  the  greater  number  of  bumble  bees  found  in  the  latter. 

A  brief  discussion  of  the  relative  importance  of  the  various  groups  con- 
cerned as  determined  by  our  studies  follows.  Owing  to  limitations  of  time, 
equipment,  personnel,  and  to  constant  trouble  from  poisoning,  much  of  the  work 
could  not  be  carried  out  as  originally  conceived  and  some  of  our  conclusions 
•must  be  regarded  as  tentative. 

2.   HIVE    BEES 

It  may  be  pointed  out  at  the  outset  that  the  question  of  orchard  pollination 
by  hive  bees  in  the  Annapolis  valley  is  at  present  largely  an  academic  one.  Due 
to  the  widespread  effect  of  poisoning  from  orchard  dusts  and  sprays,  which  is 
■discussed  in  detail  elsewhere,  the  hive  bee  population,  over  square  miles  of  the 
main  orchard  area  is  practically  nil,  and  this  area  must  depend  upon  wild  forms 
for  pollination. 

In  1931  there  was  an  aggregate  of  493  colonies  in  the  whole  of  Kings 
county,  distributed  among  46  owners.  Of  these  198  were  the  property  of  owners 
who  practiced  migratory  beekeeping;  188  were  the  property  of  the  Experiment 
Station  and  the  Pollination  Project  and  used  for  experimental  purposes;  and 
about  25  were  in  towns  or  outside  the  fruit  belt,  leaving  only  about  102  colonies 
to  pollinate  about  30,000  acres  of  orchard.  Obviously  the  hive  bee  at  the  present 
time  and  for  several  years  past  has  had  little  influence  in  fruit  pollination  in 
the  area  studied. 

3.  BUMBLE   BEES 

Bumble  bees  are  a  variable  quantity.  They  are  more  numerous  in  the 
region  of  the  North  Mountain,  and  especially  in  certain  seasons,  as  in  1930, 
were  a  decided  factor  in  pollination  of  an  orchard  at  Blomidon,  but,  in  1931  were 
much  less  numerous.  In  1932  there  was  an  apparent  increase  at  some  points, 
but,  taking  the  area  as  a  whole,  they  cannot  be  considered  an  important  factor 
in  apple  pollination.  There  is  considerable  testimony  to  the  effect  that  the 
bumble  bee  population  has  declined  in  recent  years.  Formerly,  it  is  said  that 
they  were  common  in  more  or  less  damp  meadows  where  hand  mowing  had 
to  be  resorted  to,  but  are  now  much  less  frequently  found,  especially  in  the 
Yalley  proper.  Whether  this  is  actually  the  case,  and  whether,  if  true,  it  is 
•due  to  limitation  of  breeding  places,  poisoning,  or  some  other  factor,  cannot  now 
be  determined. 


94 

The  following  is  a  list  of  the  species  taken  at  apple  bloom: — 
Br  emus  vagans  Smith  Br  emus  fervidus  Fab. 

"        terricola  Kirby  "        ternarius  Say 

"        borealis  Kirbv 

4.  SOLITARY  BEES 

By  far  the  greatest  number  of  visitors  to  apple  bloom  in  the  area  studied 
belong  to  the  genera  Halictus  and  Andrena.  The  following  is  a  partial  list  of  the 
species  taken: — 

Halictus  smilacince  Robt.  Andrena  carlini  Ckll 

"         era terus  Low  "         wilkella  Kirfcy 

"         lerouxiiLeP.  "         cratcegi  Robt. 

"         arcuatus  Robt.  "         rugosa  Robt. 

"         cressoni  Robt  "         milwaukeensis  Graen. 

"         provancheriD.T.  "         bradleyi  Vier. 

"         pilosus  Smith  "         weedi  Vier. 

"         planatusljov.  "         miranda  Sm. 

"         /ozwRobt.  "         vicina  Smith 

"        pectoralis  Smith  "         thaspii  Graen. 

li         coriaceus  Smith 

"         versans  Lov. 

Of  the  foregoing  the  first  three  species  are  probably  most  generally  abundant 
and  of  these  H.  smilacince  Robt.  far  outnumbers  all  others  and  is  probably  more 
important  in  apple  pollination  in  the  Annapolis  valley  than  all  others  combined. 
Next  to  these  in  numbers  observed  on  bloom  would  come  Andrena  carlini  Ckll. 
and  Andrena  wilkella  Kirby,  the  latter  common  everywhere,  but  particularly  so 
on  Long  island. 

It  has  been  confidently  stated  by  many  writers  that  modern  methods  of 
culture  have  reduced  the  nesting  places  for  bees,  which  fact  is  said  to  account 
for  their  scarcity.  It  is  of  interest  to  examine  this  statement  in  the  light  of  the 
information  gained  during  this  investigation  regarding  the  habits  of  the  species 
concerned.  All  are  ground-nesting  species,  living  in  tunnels  which  they  dig  in  the 
earth  and  provision  with  pellets  of  pollen  mixed  with  nectar,  on  which  the  eggs 
are  laid  and  upon  which  the  young  feed  and  develop.  In  the  case  of  Halicti 
studied  the  males  occur  in  the  late  summer  and  autumn  and  only  the  fertilized 
female  winters  over.  With  the  Andrena?  studied,  the  males  occur  in  the  spring. 
The  males,  however,  are  of  little  significance  in  pollination. 

In  the  case  of  H.  smilacince  Robt.,  the  most  abundant  and  widespread  of  all 
the  species  taken,  the  holes  may  be  found  scattered  in  various  places.  Some  are 
found  on  exposed  surfaces;  in  other  cases  the  holes  arc  partly  covered  with 
vegetation.  In  favoured  situations  they  may  be  grouped  together  in  considerable 
numbers  as  on  roadside  banks.  They  are  common  along  orchard  roads  if  not  too 
much  shaded.  They  seem  to  prefer  for  this  purpose  a  sandy  loam  containing  a 
considerable  intermixture  of  silt  and  clay. 

The  nests  of  H.  craterus  Lov.  are  found  in  many  different  types  of  situations, 
but  are  particularly  abundant  in  sparsely  covered  pastures,  drier  part  of  dyke 
lands,  etc.  H.  lerouxii  LeP.  nests  are  also  very  widespread,  but  were  found  in 
thousands  in  the  "  running  dyke  "  at  Grand  Pre.  The  nesting  habits  of  H. 
arcuatus  Robt.  arc  particularly  interesting.  It  was  found  nesting  only  in  one 
location,  but  here  it  occurred  in  countless  thousands.  This  community  was 
found  on  the  steep  slope  of  a  pasture  on  the  side  of  the  North  Mountain 
near  Centreville.  The  soil  was  classified  as  sandy  loam  but  contained  a  con- 
siderable intermixture  of  clay.  The  site  was  overgrown  with  wild  grasses,  asters, 
golden-rods,  thistles  and  other  plants  common  to  such  situations.  These  plants 
grew  largely  in  clumps,  leaving  patches   of   bare  ground  between    and   small 


95 


(»  5b 


^% 


It-  '■'•II 


\/8L 


o  0 


96 

boulders  up  tcKhe  size  of  a  football  were  scattered  over  and  through  the  ground. 
A  shovelful  of  earth  from  this  area  might  contain  scores  of  adult  bees,  larvae 
and  pellets,  and  on  a  hot  day  when  activity  was  at  its  height  the  face  of  the  bank 
was  reminiscent  of  the  front  of  a  bee-hive. 

Other  species  are  also  associated  more  or  less  with  a  certain  type  of  location. 
For  example,  Andrena  wilkella  Kirby  though  widely  abundant,  is  particularly 
associated  with  dvke  lands. 


Fig.  33. 


Running  dyke"  at  Grand  Pre.     A  favourite  nesting  place  of  Halictus  lerou.rii 
LeP.  and  other  bees    (original). 


Without  pursuing  this  subject  further,  it  may  be  pointed  out  that  roadside 
banks,  pastures  and  dykes  do  not  represent  exactly  wild  conditions,  but  are  the 
product  of  human  activity.  However,  neither  are  such  locations  intensively 
cultivated.  Cultivated  land  and  certain  soil  types,  such  as  light  sand  or  gravel, 
are  not  suited  to  nesting,  which  is  one  reason  that  the  solitary  bees  are  more 
numerous  in  such  places  as  Long  island  and  along  the  North  Mountain,  thai: 
they  are  at  many  points  situated  in  the  middle  of  the  Valley. 

The  results  of  a  more  extensive  study  of  the  biology  and  classification  of  the 
solitary  bees  concerned  in  apple  pollination,  carried  out  in  connection  with  this 
investigation  by  Mr.  C.  E.  Atwood,  will  appear  elsewhere. 


5.   FLIES   AND    OTHER   INSECTS 

Large  numbers  of  Diptera,  particularly  Syrphidae,  have  been  taken  from 
apple  bloom,  in  numbers  intermediate  between  those  of  solitary  bees  and  bumble 
bees,  but  neither  their  structure  nor  habits  lend  themselves,  to  the  same  extent 
as  bees,  for  cross-pollination  purposes.     We  are  convinced  that,  outside  the  two 


97 


;  '...."*.■ 

HHBr 

Fig.  34. — Bank  by  roadside  containing  numerous  nests  of  Ealictus  smilacinae  Robt. 

(original). 


fi 


60796—7 


Fig.  35. — Well  trodden  foot  path.     Ealictus  foxii  Robt.  was  nesting  here    (original). 


Fig.  36.— A  past 


ith   bare   patches  between  tufts  of   sen 
of  Halictug  craterus  Low   (original 


nesting  place 


m    * 


#~ 


-*)»** 

.*§: 


r 


*a»*  y? 


e? 


JtiRMhtfBW 


ated  by  Halictus  arcuatua  Robt.  (original 


99 


4 


I 


W 


m 
HOT 


f 


8 


^mar 


r 


.V 


10 


Fig.  38. — Some  species  of  bees  involved  in  pollina 
tion  of  the  apple:  (1)  Halictus  smilacinae  Robt. 
(2)  H.  provancheri  D.T.;  (3)  H.  arcuntus  Robt. 
(4)  Andrena  crataegi  Robt.;  (5)  A.  earlini  Ckll. 
(6)  Halictus  lerouxii  LeP;  (7)  H.  coriaceous 
Smith,  (8)  Andrena  wilkella  Kirby;  (9)  Bremus 
ternarius  Say;  (10)  Andrena  mihvaukeensis 
Graen.;  ((11)  A.  vicina  Smith;  (12)  Bremus  fer- 
vidus  Fab.    (original). 

genera  mentioned,  other  insects  play  a  minor  role,  and  for  this  reason  they  have 
received  little  attention  in  these  studies.  The  following  are  among  the  Diptera* 
taken  on  apple  bloom: — 

Bombylius  pygmatus  Fab.  Syrphus  torvus  0.  S. 

B.  major  L.  Cartosyrphus  slossonae  Shann. 

Eristalis  arbustorum  L.  Sphecomyia  vittata  Wied. 

E.  bastardi  Macq.  Sericomyia  militaris  Walk. 

E.  compactus  W.  Odontomyia  interrupta  Oliv. 

Rhingia  nasica  Say  Hylemya  sp. 

Melanostoma  pictipes  Big.  Brachyopa  perplexa  Curran 

Syrphus  wiedemanni  Johns.  Pollenia  rudis  F. 

S.  rectus  0.  S.  Mericia  ampelus  Walk. 

S.  amalopis  O.  S.  Criorhina  badia  Walk. 

In  addition  to  the  foregoing,  insects  of  other  families  and  orders,  as  noted 
by  other  workers  quoted,  were  also  observed  by  us.  Since  their  practical  im- 
portance as  pollinators  of  the  orchard  is  quite  insignificant,  they  have,  however, 
been  given  no  special  attention. 

*  Determined  by  Mr.  C.  H.  Curran. 
60796—71 


100 

D.   RELATIVE   VALUE   OF   INSECT   POLLINATORS 

1.  GENERAL 

In  view  of  the  situation  that  exists  in  the  area  studied,  it  was  important  to 
determine  whether  the  wild  bee  population  was  adequate  to  ensure  pollination 
under  all  conditions.  This  involved  the  working  out  of  a  method  of  determining 
the  bee  population  or,  at  least,  the  effective  pollinating  population;  and  it  also 
involved  a  comparison  of  the  various  wild  bee  pollinators  with  the  hive  bee  with 
respect  to  the  various  factors  affecting  their  value  as  pollinators  of  the  apple. 
In  this  connection  it  was  not  considered  necessary  to  embark  upon  a  study  of 
the  very  complicated  problems  dealing  with  the  response  of  the  species  con- 
cerned to  colour,  odour,  form,  etc.  Since  only  a  single  plant  species  was  involved 
the  problem  was  considerably  simplified,  and  the  limitations  of  time  and  expert 
assistance  precluded  any  particular  attention  being  given  to  many  lines  of  work, 
which,  while  of  great  scientific  interest,  were  not  strictly  necessary  to  the  main 
project.  Similar  limitations  compelled  us  to  lump  together  all  solitary  bees  under 
a  single  heading  and  treat  them  as  if  they  represented  a  single  component.  While 
it  is  recognized  that  differences  exist  among  the  species  concerned,  with  respect 
to  habit  and  value  as  apple  pollinators,  no  other  course  was  possible,  nor  is  it 
likely  that  it  would  have  affected  the  practical  results  of  our  studies. 

2.  METHOD   OF  STUDY 

One  of  the  most  important  practical  difficulties  was  in  devising  a  ready 
method,  applicable  to  work  in  the  field,  for  estimating  what  we  have  called 
"  effective  population,"  that  is,  the  bee  population  available  for  pollinating  pur- 
poses. 

(a)  ESTIMATION    OF    EFFECTIVE    POPULATION 

In  order  to  estimate  the  relative  numbers  of  different  insects  present  in  the 
bloom,  a  definite  plan  was  adopted  and  carried  out  throughout  the  entire  series 
of  investigations.  Workers  were  stationed  at  different  points  in  the  orchard  and 
made  ten  minute  counts  of  the  numbers  visiting  the  bloom  during  that  period; 
250  blossoms  representing  the  unit  of  observation.  The  observations  were  then 
recorded,  together  with  the  records  of  temperature,  humidity,  wind  velocity,  sun- 
light, etc.,  prevailing  at  that  time.  When  studies  of  distribution  were  being  made, 
for  example,  a  certain  number  of  counters  were  placed  at  suitable  intervals 
throughout  the  orchard  and  left  there  throughout  the  day,  while  others  "  scouted  " 
the  outskirts  to  secure  the  limits  of  the  flight.  (Each  counter  took  up  6  different 
points  during  the  hour  so  as  to  equalize  variations  from  more  or  less  favourably 
situated  limbs.)  At  Kentville  one  man  was  kept  at  this  work  throughout  the 
entire  blooming  period  each  year,  taking  observations  on  different  varieties  as 
they  became  attractive. 

In  utilizing  the  figures  obtained  in  these  counts  as  a  basis  for  calculation,  we 
determined  the  average  number  of  blossoms  present  per  acre  of  bearing  trees  in 
full  bloom,  by  first  tagging  a  representative  number  of  limbs,  determining  the 
percentage  that  set  fruit,  securing  the  total  crop  obtained  and,  from  this,  com- 
puting the  number  of  blossoms  that  must  have  been  present  to  produce  this  crop. 
From  this  it  was  possible  to  calculate  the  approximate  number  of  bees  per  tree 
or  per  acre  represented  by  any  count.  Thus  one  bee  taken  at  a  single  count 
would  represent  approximately  4,000  bees  per  acre  present  in  an  orchard  with 
trees  all  in  full  bloom  at  one  time. 

These  figures  approach  accuracy  in  so  far  as  the  stations  chosen  represent 
typical  conditions  existing  throughout  the  entire  orchard.  Naturally,  it  would 
be  difficult  to  ensure  that  the  stations  chosen  were  absolutely  representative. 
but  the  comparative  results  for  hive  and  wild  bees  are  based  on  identical  con- 


101 


102 

ditions  and  should  be  indicative.  Furthermore,  the  consistency  of  the  results 
obtained,  when  checked  by  various  methods,  indicates  that  in  estimating  the 
effective  population  the  foregoing  method  is  decidedly  useful  for  comparative 
purposes  and  the  results  should  be  regarded  mainly  from  this  standpoint. 

(b)  OBSERVATION  STATIONS 

Observation  "  stations  "  from  which  our  studies  of  bee  activity  could  be 
carried  out  were  selected  in  most  cases,  with  a  view  to  their  geographical  isola- 
tion. Complete  isolation  could  not  be  obtained,  though,  in  one  case,  practical 
isolation  was  secured.  Most  of  the  orchards  were  chosen  from  positions  near 
the  North  Mountain,  where  "  coves  "  running  into  the  mountain  offered  partial 
isolation  and  at  least  prevented  the  bees  from  flying  northward.  Nine  such 
sites  were  selected,  but  our  main  observations  were  carried  out  at  four  of  these 
stations,  with  a  fifth  in  1932.  Brief  descriptions  of  these  are  given,  which  with 
accompanying  maps  and  photographs  should  give  a  reasonably  clear  idea  of  the 
situation  of  the  orchard  and  the  character  of  the  territory  concerned. 

Station  No.  1,  Experimental  Station.— Including  a  small  adjacent  orchard, 
there  is  available  here  about  70  acres  of  orchard  practically  in  a  block.  It  is 
partially  isolated  as  far  as  effective  bee  flight  is  concerned,  from  other  apple 
bloom  by  ravines,  belts  of  trees,  etc.,  on  all  sides  except  the  north,  where  the 
bees  can  fly  across  the  Cornwallis  river,  to  the  large  orchards  on  the  other  side, 
distant  about  a  mile.  That  they  actually  do  this  when  the  bees  are  placed  at 
the  north  of  the  property  adjacent  to  the  post  road,  has  been  repeatedly 
observed.  Until  1931,  colonies  were  distributed  through  the  orchards,  about  50 
being  available,  and  these  were  weakened  with  poison.  Instead  of  being  placed 
one  in  a  place  as  in  former  years,  in  1931  and  1932  they  were  placed  in  groups 
in  such  a  way  as  to  secure  optimum  distribution,  and  the  number  was  reduced 
to  37. 

Station  No.  2,  Lakeville. — Here  the  colonies  were  placed  in  a  solid  ten-acre 
block  of  Blenheim,  immediately  surrounded  by  orchards,  amounting  in  all  to 
about  90  acres,  about  half  the  area  being  in  orchard  within  an  area  enclosed 
by  a  line  drawn  at  one-quarter  mile  around  the  orchard  on  all  sides. 


Fig.  40. — Map  of  Long-  island  district.  Total  area.  640  acres.  Area  in  orchard.  96  acres. 
Figures  represent  stations  at  which  apiaries  are  situated:  letters  indicate  intermedi- 
ate points  at  which  counters  were  placed    (original). 


Station  No.  3,  Pereaux. — Within  a  quarter  of  a  mile  radius  from  the  colonies 
at  this  station  we  have  30  acres  of  orchard  in  a  territory  of  about  200  acres. 
The  North  Mountain  at  the  back  of  the  orchard  prevents  flight  in  this  direction. 
This  orchard  is  more  or  less  typical  of  several  others  used  in  these  investigations. 


103 


.fflF"7 


>-.%" 


I      X 


f 


WU 


:^J 


a 


W 


104 


Fig.  42.— Map  of  Lakeville  district.     Total  area  223-6  acres;  area  in  orchard  93-7  acres- 

scale  4"  =  1  mile    (original). 


Fig.  43.— Map  of  Pereaux  district.     Total  area  220  acres,  area   in 
orchard  30   acres;    scale:    4"  =  1   mile    (original). 


105 

Station  No.  4,  Long  Island — This  is  not  now  an  island  being  connected  with 
the  mainland  by  two  miles  of  dyke  land,  which  however,  affords  little  bee 
pasturage  until  the  clover  flow.  It  is  two  miles  long  and  about  J  mile  wide  at 
the  crossroad,  bearing  about  90  acres  of  orchard,  and  with  a  total  area  of  640 
acres.  In  1928,  1929  and  1930  there  was  available  at  the  east  end  of  the  island 
an  apiary  of  25  strong  colonies  situated  just  north  of  the  east  orchard,  with  a 
belt  of  trees  between  it  and  the  beach.  In  1931,  colonies  were  placed  on  the 
island  at  the  equivalent  of  about  one  per  acre  divided  into  three  lots,  one  at 
each  end  and  one  near  the  centre.  In  1932,  these  were  divided  into  six  equal 
lots  placed  equidistant  from  each  other.  The  period  of  apple  bloom  is  normally 
several  days  later  than  on  the  mainland  and  we  could  never  find  that,  at  this 
particular  period,  there  was  any  flight  off  the  island. 

Station  5,  Somerset. — This  orchard  was  selected  for  certain  studies  of  inter- 
fruitfulness,  using  bees  as  pollinators.  They  were  placed  in  an  orchard  of 
If  acres,  with  an  acreage  of  44  within  a  one-quarter  mile  radius,  and  137-72 


//ay 


//aa  mxu/s,  ope/7  f/e/a^e/c. 


/ 37.72  acre 5 ftear/aa  a/vfara' 

W///7//7  yz/77/7e  raa'/as  afap/ary 

■44-4/  acres  />ear//7&  arc/paraf 

W/S/7//7  fesn//e  raa'/us  o/ap/ary. 

/■  72  arc  res  <?rc/7ara7  tr/rere  ap/trrt/ 
/s  S/Sua/ea'. 


Fig.  44. — Map  of  Somerset  area   (original). 

acres  enclosed  in  a  half  mile  radius.  Fifty  colonies  in  good  condition  were  placed 
in  this  orchard  and  counts  were  made  in  and  around  the  orchard  for  the  two 
days  of  maximum  bloom. 


106 


107 


108 

3.  RELATIVE  ABUNDANCE  OF  HIVE  BEES  AND  WILD  BEES 

DURING  BLOOM 

The  following  important  advantage  is  claimed  for  hive  bees  and  may  here 
be  discussed  in  the  light  of  our  own  observations  and  those  of  other  workers. 
The  hive  bee  is  the  only  species  in  which  the  workers  winter  over,  and  hence 
more  individuals  are  available  for  pollination  than  in  the  case  of  wild  bees  in 
which  only  the  queens  hibernate. 

The  fact  that  only  the  hive  bees  over  winter  their  workers  is,  of  course, 
true;  and  in  view  of  the  fact  that  only  queens  among  the  wild  species  hibernate, 
the  number  of  individuals  sometimes  present  in  the  spring  is  nothing  short  of 
surprising.  Various  workers,  as  already  noted,  have  made  observations  on  this 
point,  but  only  a  few  have  attempted  actually  quantitative  determinations. 
Therefore  the  results  of  our  own  detailed  studies  on  this  point  are  of  interest. 

Some  useful  calculations  as  to  the  relative  abundance  of  hive  bees  and  wild 
bees  under  known  conditions  may  be  based  on  our  work  on  Long  island,  Kings 
county,  N.S.  At  this  station  we  had  an  excellent  opportunity  to  make  com- 
parative studies  of  the  bee  population,  both  of  the  hive  and  the  wild  species. 
Owing  to  the  isolation  of  this  area  and  the  fact  that  it  came  into  bloom  a  little 
later  than  the  mainland,  there  was  apparently  no  flight  off  the  island.  At  the 
time  of  apple  blossoming  there  was  also  available  a  certain  amount  of  blue- 
berry, rhododendron,  dandelion,  and  other  wild  flowers,  though  of  course  apple 
predominated. 

In  1931  and  1932  when  bees  were  placed  on  Long  island  at  the  rate  of  one 
colony  per  acre,  we  obtained  an  average  10-minute  count  of  1-94  bees  in  the 
first  year  and  3-05  in  the  second.  Since,  in  1932,  there  was  only  about  60  per 
cent  of  the  bloom  of  the  previous  year,  this  would  indicate  little  difference  in 
the  effective  population,  assuming  that  there  would  occur  a  greater  concentra- 
tion of  bees  on  the  smaller  number  of  blossoms.  The  solitary  bees  in  the  same 
area  were  present  in  greater  numbers  in  1931,  i.e.,  they  were  present  in  the 
bloom  in  greater  numbers  than  a  field  force  released  by  one  strong  overwintered 
colony  of  hive  bees  per  acre.  In  1932  the  number  was  less,  corresponding  to 
an  apparent  decrease  in  the  solitary  bee  fauna  from  all  stations.  This  observa- 
tion may  be  correlated  with  a  heavy  mortality  occurring  among  the  solitary 
bees  in  the  summer  of  1931,  apparently  due  to  drowning  in  the  nests  following 
wet  weather.  The  dry  summers  of  1928-1931  may  have  been  particularly 
favourable  for  the  numerical  increase  of  solitary  bees.  The  average  number 
of  wild  bees  taken  at  all  stations  for  all  years  would  indicate  an  effective  popu- 
lation equal  to  that  released  by  a  concentration  of  one  hive  bee  colony  per 
acre.  If  our  blossom  counts  truly  represent  the  facts,  they  would  indicate  that 
about  one-third*  the  field  force  of  the  colonies  was  available  for  pollination  pur- 
poses during  apple  bloom  and,  if  we  are  to  assume  that  every  solitary  bee  found 
is  potentially  a  pollinator,  then  the  "  effective  "  population  is  even  greater,  for 
not  all  hive  bees  carry  pollen.  Counts  of  7,000  bees  made  during  apple  bloom 
at  Ottawa,  showed  that  less  than  25  per  cent  were  pollen  gatherers  and  even 
if  we  consider  double  the  number  are  effective  pollinators,  these  figures  would 
appear  to  indicate  that  under  favourable  conditions  for  pollination,  the  solitary 
bee  population  available  during  the  period  under  review  is  sufficient  alone  to 
effect  the  pollination  of  the  fruit  crop. 

Records  of  activity  from  Ottawa,  Ontario,  and  Abbotsford,  Quebec,  sup- 
plied by  Mr.  C.  B.  Gooderham,  while  based  on  too  small  counts  to  permit  of 
generalization,  indicate  that,  while  hive  bees  are  present  in  great  abundance 
owing  to  the  greater  prevalence  of  beekeeping,  wild  bees,  while  less  numerous, 
were  present  in  sufficient  force  to  accomplish  pollination  under  normal  conditions. 

*  Measurements  made  by  C.  B.  Gooderham  at  Ottawa  on  3  colonies  showed  61%,  38%   and 
48%  respectively,  as  the  percentage  of  the  total  force  going  into  the  fields  during  apple  bloom. 


109 

The  fact  should  be  strongly  emphasized  that  our  studies  of  wild  bees  have 
been  carried  on  for  too  short  a  time  to  enable  us  definitely  to  state  that  they 
can  always  be  depended  upon  to  pollinate  the  orchards  in  the  area  studied.  It 
is  of  interest,  however,  to  note  that  over  a  period  of  four  years  our  counts  of 
hive  bees  from  orchards  where  they  were  placed  in  supposedly  adequate  num- 
bers to  effect  pollination,  averaged  2-34  per  count;  and  wild  bees  1-42  per  count, 
which,  even  allowing  a  much  greater  proportion  of  effective  pollinators  for  the 
hive  bee  than  our  counts  indicate,  still  leaves  a  comfortable  margin  in  favour 
of  the  solitary  bees  so  far  as  mere  numbers  of  pollinators  are  concerned.  That 
each  solitary  bee  will  pollinate  as  many  blossoms  as  a  hive  bee,  however,  can- 
not be  definitely  stated,  but  is  discussed  in  another  section. 

TABLE  No.  16— AVERAGE  NUMBER  OF  HIVE  AND  WILD  BEES  PER  10-MINUTE  COUNT  UNDER 
CONDITIONS  PERMITTING  FLIGHT 


Locality 

1929 

1930 

1931 

1932 

Remarks 

Hive 

Wild* 

Hive 

Wild 

Hive 

Wild 

Hive 

Wild 

Long  island 

Kentville 

9-25 
1-28 

2-75 
1-40 

1-47 
3-51 
1-15 

2-26 
1-66 
1-21 

1-94 
0-77 
106 

3-01 
1-87 
1-87 

3-05 
1-58 

1-38 
0-14 

Hive  bee  counts  not  comparable 
in  different  years,  due  to  varying 
number  of  colonies  used.  In 
1931  and  1932  equivalent  to  one 
colony  per  acre.  More  effective 
distribution  in  1932. 

Counts  omitted  first  four  days  and 
last  two  days  of  bloom  in  1931. 
Thirty-seven  colonies  for  70 
acres  of  orchard  in  1931  and  1932, 
all  weakened  by  poison,  especi- 
ally in  1931. 

Forty  colonies  placed  in  a  40-acre 
Blenheim  orchard  in  1931.  Only 
16  colonies  available  in  1930. 

Fifty  colonies  placed  in  a  lf-acre 
orchard  with  137  acres  of  orchard 
available  within  one  half-mile. 

5-52 
2-83 
5-05 

1-40 
0-66 
1-36 

of  orchard  within  one  quarter 
mile.     Other  bloom  scanty. 

and  one-half  acres. 

*"Wild"  bees  refers  only  to  solitary  bees,  not  to  bumble  bees. 

4.    ARTIFICIAL    INCREASE    AND    DISTRIBUTION 

Another  advantage  claimed  for  the  hive  bee  as  a  pollinator  is  as  follows:  — 
Hive  bees  only  can  be  artificially  increased  and  evenly  distributed  in  the 
orchards.  This  is  a  decided  point  for  the  hive  bee  as  an  orchard  pollinator. 
There  is  no  method  of  artificially  increasing  the  population  of  wild  solitary  bees 
such  as  Halicti  and  Andrenae  should  their  numbers  become  depleted  from  any 
cause,  and,  while  investigators  have  succeeded  in  "  domesticating  "  bumble  bees, 
no  one  has  ever  suggested  a  method  of  carrying  it  out  on  a  commercial  scale. 
On  the  other  hand,  bee  colonies  can  be  obtained  by  rental  or  purchase  and 
furnish  a  ready  method  of  providing  orchard  pollinators,  where  observation 
indicates  a  shortage  of  wild  species.  Furthermore,  they  can  be  so  placed  as 
to  secure  efficient  distribution.  In  addition  they  can  be  moved  about  at  will 
as  needed. 


5.  COMPARATIVE   EFFECT   OF   CLIMATIC   FACTORS   ON   WILD   AND 

HIVE  BEES 

A  number  of  workers  have  discussed  the  general  effects  of  climate,  only  a 
few  of  which  can  be  noted  at  this  point. 

In  discussing  the  effects  of  temperature  on  bees,  Phillips  (1927)  points  out 
that  body  temperature  is  the  same  as  or  slightly  higher  than  that  of  the  sur- 


110 

rounding  air,  though  in  flight  it  is  considered  that  it  is  a  few  degrees  higher. 
During  the  active  season  bees  may  remain  away  from  the  hive  all  night,  a  fact 
also  noted  in  our  studies. 

Studies  were  made  by  Hutson  (1926)  as  to  the  activity  of  honey  bees  in 
orchards  at  blossoming  time  and  the  factors  influencing  it,  especially  with  refer- 
ence to  the  effect  of  hive  placing  and  weather.  The  factors  directly  affecting 
honey  bee  flight  were  said  to  be  (1)  temperature,  (2)  sunlight,  (3)  moisture  and 
(4)  air  movement.  Temperature  was  stated  not  to  have  been  a  large  factor  in 
honeybee  flight  in  these  studies.  Little  influence  was  exerted  by  humidity,  short 
of  actual  precipitation,  which  stopped  flight.  Sunlight  was  said  to  favourably 
affect  flight,  but  did  not  urge  bees  into  the  air  if  other  conditions  were  unfavour- 
able. Wind  proved  an  important  factor  in  honeybee  flight  in  these  studies,  little 
flight  taking  place  when  the  wind  was  above  20  miles  per  hour.  This  worker 
and  most  others  have  given  the  major  part  of  their  attention  to  the  activity  of 
the  hive  bee. 

According  to  Phillips  (1930)  the  flight  of  bees  is  limited  by  temperature, 
wind  and  moisture.  He  states  that,  at  60°  short  flights  are  possible,  free  flight 
taking  place  at  65°  and  full  flight  at  70°  F.,  while  some  wild  bees,  especially 
bumble  bees,  fly  at  a  lower  temperature.  He  notes  that  a  wind  of  25  miles 
per  hour  stops  bee  flight,  lower  velocities  greatly  retard  it.  Sunshine  is  not 
necessary  for  flight,  but  cloudy  weather  keeps  the  bees  near  the  hive  by  con- 
fining them  to  short  flights.  He  also  states  that,  even  in  summer,  a  sudden 
shower  will  reduce  the  day's  flight  by  10  per  cent  or  more,  while  rain  or  mist 
stops  flight. 

DeOng  (1925)  gives  tables  to  show  that  rain  stops  the  flight  of  honey  bees, 
even  at  temperatures  of  52°  to  70°  F.,  while  on  clear  days  the  bees  work  at  all 
temperatures  above  48°  F.  He  believes  that  these  figures  probably  apply  to 
all  bees. 

Marshall,  Johnson,  et  al.  (1929)  state  that  bees  are  most  active  on  bright 
warm  days,  that  they  do  not  fly  readily  at  temperatures  below  52-56°  F..  and 
that  a  wind  velocity  of  20  miles  or  more  per  hour  is  unfavourable  to  their 
activity.  They  state  that  bees  prefer  to  fly  against  the  wind  when  moving  to 
the  field,  and  with  the  wind  when  returning  with  their  load.  Though  they  may 
travel  a  considerable  distance  in  good  weather,  their  flight  is  limited  to  a  few 
hundred  yards  during  bad  weather,  according  to  these  workers. 

Lundie  (1925)  in  a  survey  of  the  total  daily  exits  and  returns  for  the  period 
of  the  observations,  found  that  a  factor  or  group  of  factors  can  reduce  the  total 
number  of  possible  exits  by  an  amount  varying  from  total  prohibition  of  flight 
to  a  fraction  of  1  per  cent.  A  threatening  storm,  for  instance,  of  but  one  hour's 
duration,  reduced  the  possible  flight  on  one  day  in  the  honey  flow  by  7-41  to 
9-67  per  cent. 

Comparatively  few  data  were  obtained  by  this  investigator  on  the  effect 
of  wind  on  the  flights.  On  one  day,  however,  a  wind  velocity  of  16  to  21  miles 
per  hour,  during  the  hours  9  a.m.  to  6  p.m.,  reduced  the  possible  maximum  flight 
by  28-53  per  cent. 

Under  a  particular  set  of  conditions,  the  temperature  at  which  the  day's 
flight  commences  was  found  to  be  uniformly  near  a  certain  definite  temperature; 
but  this  definite  temperature  is  not  always  the  same.  In  April  it  was  from  12° 
to  14°  C.  and  in  May  from  16°  to  18°  C.  On  dull  days  this  temperature  was 
usually  2°  higher.  The  internal  conditions  of  the  colony  govern  this  temperature 
somewhat,  a  strong  colony  commencing  flight  at  a  lower  temperature  than  does 
a  weak  one. 

There  was  a  considerable  variation  in  the  hour  and  temperature  at  which 
the  peak  of  the  flight  in  the  honey  flow  occurs.  No  conclusive  evidence  has 
been  obtained  that  under  similar  conditions  a  good  honey  flow  induces  the  bees 
to  go  out  in  large  numbers  at  a  lower  temperature  than  they  would  if  no  nectar 


Ill 

were  available.  The  temperature  at  which  the  flights  in  the  evening  began 
to  slacken  was,  without  exception,  from  1°  to  9°  C.  higher  than  the  tempera- 
ture at  which  flight  began  in  the  morning.  Days  which  appear  to  be  similar 
in  every  respect,  but  which  show  a  variation  of  as  much  as  from  10  to  25  per 
cent  in  their  total  flights,  are  found  to  differ  on  account  of  a  lower  temperature 
in  the  early  part  of  the  day. 

Observations  by  Woodrow  (1932)  during  apple  bloom,  indicate  that  the 
temperature  of  greatest  activity  may  differ  in  different  types  of  colonies.  Flight 
began  at  lower  temperatures  and  was  proportionally  much  heavier  in  the  lower 
temperature  ranges  from  the  stronger  colonies;  but,  at  the  higher  temperature 
ranges,  there  was  a  proportionally  heavier  flight  from  the  weaker  colonies.  The 
point  of  greatest  activity  was  reached  at  76-78  degrees  F.  for  a  colony  containing 
1-63  pounds  of  adult  bees;  at  82-84  degrees  F.  for  an  8-25-pound  colony 
but  there  was  little  significant  difference  in  this  colony  from  76-78  degrees 
upward.  Indeed,  from  62-64  degrees  upward  there  is  considerable  overlapping 
in  the  number  of  workers  leaving  the  hive.  For  example,  the  average  flight 
at  this  range  was  206-25  bees  per  minute,  compared  with  202-67  at  78-80 
degrees  F.  This  would  seem  to  indicate  that  the  maximum  flights  should  occur 
in  strong  colonies  from  60°  F.  upward,  but  in  weak  colonies  such  flights  would 
not  be  expected  until  over  72°  F. 

MacDaniels  (1930)  considers  wind  velocity  the  greatest  limiting  factor  of 
pollination  in  western  New  York.  Park  (1923)  notes  that  bees  make  little 
progress  against  a  wind  of  more  than  15  miles  per  hour. 

The  period  of  apple  bloom  occurs  at  a  time  of  uncertain  weather.  Low 
temperature,  rains,  cloudy  weather,  and  such  conditions  are  sometimes  respon- 
sible for  low  crop  yields,  through  inhibiting  the  flight  of  insect  pollinators,  as 
well  as  creating  generally  conditions  unsuitable  for  pollination.  The  following 
data  collected  during  apple  bloom,  represent  observations  over  such  a  short 
period  that  they  should  be  studied  in  connection  with  the  supplementary  data 
obtained  during  the  golden-rod  flow  later  in  the  season. 

(a)  TEMPERATURE 

With  respect  to  temperature,  the  following  claim  has  been  made  on  behalf 
of  the  hive  bee: 

Hive  bees  work  at  lower  temperatures  than  do  wild  solitary  bees.  It  should 
be  understood  that  our  data  relate  entirely  to  bee  activity  in  apple  bloom. 
Frequently,  bees  were  noticed  working  on  dandelions  and  other  low  growing 
plants  when  none  could  be  detected  in  the  trees. 


»      3i      52     35     54     S3     56     57     58     59     60     61      62     65     64     65     66     67     66     69     70     71       7E75     74737677T»79808I8E85*48586     87» 


Fig.   47. — Relation   of  hive   bee   activity  in   apple   bloom   to   temperature,    1929-1932    (original) 


So  far  as  this  factor  influences  activity  in  apple  bloom,  the  optimum  for 
both  the  hive  and  the  wild  species,  confining  the  latter  term  to  Halictus  and 
Andrena  and  not  including  bumble  bees,  on  the  basis  of  our  observations, 
appears  to  be  nearly  the  same,  viz.,  about  68°  F.  for  wild  bees  and  about  67°  F. 
for  hive  bees.  It  should  be  noted  that  in  our  counts  a  greater  number  of  observa- 
tions were  made  at  the  lower  ranges  of  temperature.     Hence  our  averages  for 


112 

these  ranges  are  based  on  a  larger  number  of  counts  and  this  no  doubt  affected 
the  point  of  apparent  optimum  activity.  Had  higher  temperatures  throughout 
the  bloom  been  the  rule  rather  than  the  exception,  the  point  of  apparent  optimum 
activity  might  have  been  higher.  The  results  of  other  workers  already  noted, 
and  of  supplementary  observations  made  on  this  point,  and  presented  later  in 
this  report,  indicate  that  this  is  the  case.  At  the  same  time  it  should  be  borne 
in  mind  that  the  temperature  of  greatest  activity  may  be  lower  early  in  the  sea- 
son, as  noted  by  Lundie  (1925).  Observations  made  throughout  the  entire  day 
indicate  that  the  hive  bees  begin  to  work  earlier  in  the  morning  and  usually 
persist  later  in  the  day. 

Above  the  temperature  indicated  they  worked  well  up  to  80°  F.,  but  the 
number  found  at  work  over  90°  F.  was  insignificant.  It  should  be  said,  how- 
ever, that  such  high  temperatures  very  seldom  occur  during  apple  bloom. 
Small  numbers  were  found  at  work  at  temperatures  as  low  as  50°  F.,  i.e.,  actual 
orchard  temperature,  and  rarely  at  lower  temperatures;  and,  with  all  other 
conditions  favourable,  fair  activity  was  observed  at  temperatures  as  low  as 
57°  F.  A  temperature  of  65°  F.  is  usually  given  as  the  minimum  effective 
temperature,  but  we  would  be  inclined  to  place  this  figure  somewhat  lower,  pro- 
vided other  factors  were  favourable.  With  reference  to  the  effect  of  temperature 
on  the  distance  of  flight  from  the  colonies,  the  statement  of  other  workers  that 
only  short  flights  are  made  during  the  lower  temperature  ranges,  as  is  also  the 
case  with  other  unfavourable  factors,  would  appear  to  be  well  substantiated.  At 
65°  F.,  however,  there  appears  to  be  no  evidence  of  flight  limitation,  and  at 
70°  F.  maximum  flights  are  to  be  expected. 

Hive  bees  do  not  appear  to  be  as  sensitive  to  slight  changes  :n  temperature  as 
do  many  of  the  wild  species,  and  a  larger  proportion  is  found  at  the 
lower  temperatures.  They  also  seem  to  be  more  erratic  in  their  response  to 
temperature  during  their  work  in  the  bloom  than  do  the  wild  species.  This  is 
brought  out  in  the  accompanying  figure  which  shows  the  curve  for  wild  bee 
activity  more  consistent  than  that  for  hive  bees.  This  is  not  believed  to  be  due 
entirely  to  errors  in  methods  of  sampling,  since  the  same  method  was 
used  for  both  wild  and  hive  bees.  It  may  be  that  hive  bees  are  more 
sensitive  to  other  influences  which  modify  temperature  effects.  They  appear,  for 
example,  to  show  a  greater  tendency  to  concentrate  in  locations  where  bloom  is  in 
a  particularly  favourable  condition.  Another  factor  that  appears  to  make  the 
temperature  response  somewhat  erratic  is  the  fact  that  the  sudden  breaking 
away  of  clouds,  clearing  up  of  a  fog  or  similar  condition,  causes  the  hive  bees 
to  come  out  with  a  rush,  thus  giving  what  appear  to  be  abnormally  high 
counts  for  those  periods.  The  modifying  effect  of  light  conditions  is  discussed  in 
the  next  section.  It  is  clear  from  these  observations  that  our  counts  fall  off 
rapidly  as  the  lower  light  readings  are  recorded  in  the  late  afternoon,  even  though 
temperature  conditions  remain  constant  or  recede  at  a  much  less  rapid  rate.  In 
fact,  in  some  cases  we  have  reductions  in  numbers  of  bees  even  with  a  tempera- 
ture rising  to  an  apparently  more  favourable  point.  Our  records  show  an  appar- 
ent difference  in  temperature  response  between  morning  and  afternoon,  higher 
values  being  indicated  in  the  morning  readings,  which  indicate  that  the  effect  of 
each  degree  of  temperature,  within  a  giveD  range,  is  of  varying  value,  depending 
on  light  conditions.  Temperature — conditioned  by  light  and  modified  also  by 
wind,  nectar  secretion,  pollen  availability,  colony  strength  and  similar  condi- 
tions— is  a  crucial  factor  in  bee  activity  during  apple  bloom. 

It  should  be  emphasized,  therefore,  that  the  response  to  temperature  cannot 
be  considered  apart  from  light,  wind  and  other  factors.  Hence,  we  can  only  si  ate 
in  a  general  way  what  the  optimum  temperature  range  for  bee  activity  is.  The 
temperature  at  which  the  highest  average  number  of  bees  is  taken,  might  be  very 
different  in  different  localities,  different  seasons,  different  times  of  the  same  season. 
different  days,  or  even  different  periods  of  the  day.  owing  to  the  modifying 


113 

influence  of  light,  wind  and  other  effects.  The  peak  of  the  activity  curve,  there- 
fore, represents  the  largest  average  number  taken  at  that  particular  temperature 
during  apple  bloom  over  a  four-year  period,  but  it  does  not  necessarily  represent 
the  true  optimum  temperature  for  general  bee  activity.  Also,  the  apparent 
falling  off  in  activity  after  the  peak  may  be  attributed  to  the  comparatively  small 
number  of  observations  made  at  the  higher  temperatures.  During  golden-rod 
flow,  which  occurred  at  a  time  when  higher  temperatures  were  the  rule,  there  was 
a  steady  increase  in  activity  up  to  84°  F.,  which  was  the  highest  observed  temper- 
ature. 

Lundie's  (1925)  statement  that  the  temperature  at  which  flight  begins  to 
slacken  in  the  afternoon  is  from  1°  to  9°  C.  higher  than  that  at  which  flight 
begins  in  the  morning,  seems,  in  general,  to  be  substantiated  by  our  figures  and  is 
true  for  both  hive  and  wild  bees.  This  would  appear  to  be  a  light  effect,  as  brought 
out  in  another  section. 

(b)  SUNDIGHT  AND  SOLAR  RADIATION 

Sunlight  is  an  important  factor  in  influencing  the  activity  of  bees,  but  sur- 
light  alone  will  not  cause  them  to  work  provided  the  temperature  is  too  low. 
An  interesting  effect  of  lack  of  sunlight  is  noticed  on  shaded  limbs,  the  blossoms 
on  which  fail  to  be  pollinated  to  a  much  greater  extent  than  on  limbs  exposed 
to  sunlight.  Bees  respond  to  sunlight  very  rapidly,  as  can  readily  be  observed  in 
weather  that  is  partly  cloudy.  It  was  noted  that  counts  made,  even  at  optimum 
temperatures,  showed  fewer  bees  present  in  hazy  weather,  even  without  definite 
cloud  banks,  than  when  the  sky  was  clear.  The  rapidity  with  which  bees  return 
to  the  bloom  following  bursts  of  sunlight  is  noteworthy. 


RELATIVE  RESPONSE  OF  CELL  AHD  FILTERS 

3.26/77.777.  RED  ~  2.4-3      ) 

3. 78 777. 777.  GREEN* 401  I  CORNING  GLASS  CO. 

3.29/77.777.  BLUE ~ 554  \ 

4.7.1mm.  U-V     ^9B6J 

AND  WESTON  "PHOTRON/C  CELL  WITH  QUARTZ  COVER 


Fig.  48.— Relative  response  of  cell  and  niters  of  solar  radiation  apparatus    (original) 
60796—8 


114 


The  stimulative  effect  of  ultra-violet  light  on  insects  has  been  demonstrated 
by  a  number  of  workers  who  have  investigated  the  "  colour  sense  "  in  this  class, 
and  several  of  these  have  used  the  hive  bee  in  their  experiments.  This  work 
has  mainly  concerned  the  attraction  of  different  coloured  flowers  for  the  insects. 
It  was  thought  that  something  might  be  learned  from  studying  the  distribution 
of  solar  radiation  throughout  the  day  in  relation  to  the  activity  of  hive  bees  and 
solitary  bees  in  the  bloom,  as  might  be  expected  from  the  results  of  Bertholf 
(1931  and  1931a)  and  others. 

Through  the  kindness  of  Mr.  J.  Patterson,  Director  of  the  Meteorological 
Service  of  Canada,  who  provided  us  with  the  necessary  equipment  for  this  work 
and  assisted  us  by  means  of  advice,  we  were  enabled  to  give  this  problem  some 
attention  during  the  period  of  apple  bloom  in  1932. 

The  accompanying  figure  illustrates  the  relative  response  of  the  cell  and 
filters,  and  the  following  table  shows  the  percentage  radiation  falling  within 
the  indicated  bands,  which  is  effective  in  producing  the  galvanometer  deflection, 
the  shunt  for  which  was  adjusted  to  about  i  of  1  per  cent,  the  filters  calibrated 
to  within  2  per  cent: 


Filter 

Band 

Band,  per  cent 

Maximum 

per  cent  at 

U- V 

2800-3900 
6600-7800 
6600-7800 
3400-4200 
3550-4950 
4750-6150 
6080-7800 

110 
1-8 
1-6 
2-5 

19-0 
70 

190 

17 
3 
2 
4 

32 
8 

52 

3460 

U-V 

R  +  U-V 

7000 
7000 

B  +  U-V 

3700 

Blue 

4600 

Green 

Red 

5100-5400 
6250 

As  the  ultra-violet  filter  had  also  a  transmission  band  in  the  red  and  infra- 
red, it  was  considered  desirable  to  take  a  reading  with  both  the  red  and  ultra- 
violet filters  on  the  instrument,  as  well  as  with  each  of  the  filters  singly.  The 
ultra-violet  radiation  was  then  given  by  the   following  formula: 

Ultra-violet=U-V— 1  •  14  (R+U-V  I . 
This  constant  has  been  evaluated  on  the  assumption  that  there  is  a  uniform  dis- 
tribution of  radiation  between  6600  and  7800A.  As  the  distribution  on  any  occa- 
sion is  unknown,  this  assumption  was  necessary,  but  the  resulting  error  would 
be  small.  The  combination  of  blue  and  ultra-violet  filters  was  also  used,  as  this 
isolates  a  rather  narrow  band  in  the  vicinity  of  3750A. 

Half  hourly  readings  were  taken  on  a  number  of  typical  days  throughout 
the  bloom  and  curves  drawn  to  show  the  distribution  of  solar  radiation  throughout 
those  particular  days,  together  with  temperature.  Curves  have  also  been  plotted 
for  bee  activity,  based  on  average  counts  over  the  same  period.  In  order  to  allow 
all  values  to  be  placed  on  a  single  graph,  arbitrary  factors  have  been  assigned  to 
each,  as  indicated. 

Furthermore,  in  order  to  facilitate  inspection  of  graphs  indicating  bee  activity, 
the  ultra-violet  and  clear  values  are  indicated  on  a  single  chart  together  with 
temperature,  while  the  other  values  art1  placed  on  another  chart.  Since  all  the 
observations  could' not  be  taken  simultaneously,  it  was  not  possible  to  secure 
exact  synchronization  of  the  different  readings,  and  certain  irregularities,  there- 
fore, resulted. 

It  will  be  seen,  however,  that  there  is  a  general  trend  upward  of  bee  activity. 
corresponding  with  increasing  light  values,  and  a  corresponding  decrease  when 
light  readings  normally  fall  off  in  the  afternoon;  or,  from  the  effects  of  clouds. 
haze  or  fog,  at  any  time  during  the  day.  It  will  be  observed,  also,  that  within 
the  tempera t urc  range  of  bee  activity,  light  apparently  has  a  more  important 
influence  than  slight  changes  in  temperature.  It  will  be  observed  that  the  bee 
counts  fall  off'  much  more  rapidly  than  temperature  in  many  cases.     In  others 


115 

they  fall  off  with  receding  light  values  at  a  nearly  stationary  temperature  and, 
in  still  others,  there  is  actually  a  falling  off  in  activity  as  light  values  recede, 
even  with  a  slightly  rising  temperature. 

We  were  unable  by  this  method,  however,  to  secure  any  clear  indication 
that  any  particular  wave  length  had  any  greater  stimulative  effect  than  any 
other.    In  this  connection  it  has  been  determined  by  Bertholf  (loc.  cit.)  that  the 


9.00  KUDO  11.00  12.00  1.00  2.00  300  400  500 

Fig.  49. — Distribution  of  solar  radiation,  June  3.   1932    (original). 


&30    900     930     ia00    1050     1100    1130    K00   12.30     100     1.50    EDO    230    300    330  400    430    5.00     5.50   6.00    630    700    730 
Fig.  50. — Distribution  of  solar  radiation,  June  4,   1932    (original). 

maximum  stimulative  efficiency  for  the  honey  bee  in  the  non-visible  or  ultra- 
violet portion  of  the  spectrum  is  at  36oOA,  and  the  values  for  stimulative  effi- 
ciency fall  away  rapidly  on  receding  from  these  values.     The  greater  part  of 

the  response  of  our  ultra-violet  filter  No.  986  lies  in  the  region  of  3450A.  The 
same  applies  to  the  green  filter  No.  401,  where  the  maximum  response  is  in  the 

60796— 8J 


116 


region  of  5300A,  while  the  maximum  stimulative  efficiency  of  light  in  the  visible 
spectrum  (though  this  has  only  about  %  the  stimulative  effect  of  the  ultra- 
violet) is  at  5530A.  Even  though  it  were  possible  to  isolate  narrower  bands 
in  the  region  of  maximum  stimulative  efficiency  by  means  of  this  apparatus, 
it  is  not  certain  that  we  would  secure  any  further  information  in  regard  to  this 
particular  point,  because  of  the  tendency  for  all  readings  to  follow  the  same 
general  trend  throughout  the  day.  Without  an  automatic  recording  device  for 
light  values  and  bee  numbers,  the  difficulty  of  properly  synchronizing  the  two  sets 
of  observations  is  difficult  to  overcome. 


4.30 


5.50  6.50  750  8.50  9.50  10.50  11.50  IE30 

Fig.  51. — Distribution  of  solar  radiation,  June  5,  1932    (original). 


10 

/ 

\ 

i 

\ 

A 

A 

/ 

--* 

1    ' 

% 

1 

\ 

^-: 

\ 

■  X 

\ 

V 

!  J 

4 

^ 

/; 

^" 

1 

-- 

// 

1 

1 

I 

\ 

jj 

.'-- 

J* 

/ 

i 

-V 

v_ 

s\. 

< 

0     40     0. 

Va      - 

C&6 

St 

Jitnta 

^  ,.:•■    ■ 

Hr./TS 

CAsvr 

?S  i/ua 

rS/&£ 

1  < 

■  i 
|4 

| 

f 

IS 

m 

<tf-Y 

M 

c#Aw 

\ 

ZT 

fempera/t 

C/ear 

n? 

V 

0 

r" 

^ 

8 

^.,- 

\ 

.■r 

1 

^v 

\ 

\ 

«      6 

--- 

--- 

-—■~ 

^N 

N 

V 

\ 

11 

' 

v*v 

\ 

\ 

. 

/ 

y 

V* 

V 

^^ 

sx~- 

\ 

,'  1 

/- 

-~^s 

-V- 

\ 
\ 
\ 

2    60  a 

/ 

Oearr  - 

/ 

*^ 

:  / 

7 

-a^ 

'" 

^^" 

\ 

\ 

\ 

\ 
\ 

Vonq  /stentf,  As  June  J.  /9Jk 

J_LL 1 

/ 

/ 

'.otj/sArna'./rS 

June+ 

SK 

- — <s 

\ 

\ 

\ 

i 

10          5 

10         11 

00          1 

10            12 

00           1 

Time 

0             J 

00            -4 

00           J 

0            6 

o          8 

1          9 

0             11 

30           1 

90          K 

30              L 

n/nt 

it          £ 

X          S 

1         4 

So        1 

»         6 

i 

Fig.  52. — Bee  activity  in  relation  to  temperature  and  light    (original), 


117 

With  respect  to  the  differential  response  of  hive  bees  and  solitary  bees  it  is 
again  difficult  to  make  comparisons,  especially  as  there  are  unequal  numbers 
of  each  available.  There  is  a  definite  indication,  however,  based  on  numerous 
individual  observations,  that  hive  bees  actually  do  work  more  readily  at  lower 
light  values  than  do  the  solitary  bees  taken  as  a  group. 

(c)  WIND 

Wind  is  an  important  factor  in  governing  the  activity  of  bees,  as  indicated 
by  the  observations  of  other  workers  already  quoted.  Winds  likewise  limit  the 
length  of  flight  of  bees  in  the  same  way  as  unfavourable  temperature  conditions, 
so  that  they  will  be  found  nearer  the  hives  in  sheltered  areas  during  such  periods. 
In  our  counts,  the  maximum  wind  velocity  recorded  up  to  and  including  1931 
was  only  13  miles  per  hour,  and  even  at  this  velocity  there  was  considerable 
activity.  The  observation  was  made,  however,  that  on  the  eastern  end  of  Long 
island,  where  belts  of  wood  at  the  north  of  the  colonies  sheltered  them  from 
the  open  basin,  the  bees  worked  in  greatest  numbers. 


i 


K£-V 


/five  Se.  ?s 
/Y//o/3ees- 


/V//7t/Kf/0C//t/  //?  /77//<?S 


SS    t    65" 


13      2      2.5     3     3.5     4     45     5 


7      75     6     8.5     9     95      10      10.5     I      11.5      12     125 


Fig.  53. — Bee  activity  in  relation  to  wind  velocity 
(original). 


It  would  appear,  however,  that  in  the  average  year  during  the  period  of 
apple  bloom  and  in  the  situations  where  the  observations  were  made,  wind  was 
not  as  important  an  inhibiting  factor  as  reported  from  certain  other  areas.  In 
1932,  however,  apparent  cases  of  flight  inhibition  due  to  wind  were  observed. 
A  tabulation  of  the  numbers  taken  at  the  different  wind  velocities  was  made, 
which,  allowing  for  the  usual  large  number  of  apparent  irregularities  resulting 
from  errors  in  sampling,  and  from  the  widely  different  number  of  observations 
at  different  velocities,  seems  to  indicate  a  decided  influence  for  even  very  low 
velocities.  This  can  best  be  observed  by  reference  to  the  preceding  graph  which 
has  been  smoothed  by  the  method  of  moving  averages. 

There  is  no  indication  of  interference  with  flight  in  the  case  of  hive  bees 
up  to  1  m.p.h.  and  for  wild  bees  up  to  3  m.p.h.,  after  which  the  counts  began 
to  fall  off.  From  the  foregoing  it  would  seem  that,  in  locations  where  high 
winds  are  frequent,  this  factor  may  be  of  the  utmost  importance  in  pollination, 
but  our  own  records  are  inadequate  to  allow  of  any  more  definite  statement. 


118 

Our  records  would  also  seem  to  show  that  wild  bees  during  the  same  period 
appeared  to  be  somewhat  less  profoundly  affected  by  changes  in  wind  velocity 
than  are  the  hive  bees.  The  number  of  observations  taken,  however,  are  insuffi- 
cient to  enable  us  to  state  with  certainty  that  this  is  the  case. 

(d)  HUMIDITY 

Humidity,  short  of  actual  rainfall  which  checks  flight,  is  generally  regarded 
as  having  little  influence  on  bee  activity.  Bees  have  been  noted  gathering 
moisture  from  a  nearby  spruce  hedge  during  a  light  rain  and,  in  one  case,  bees 
in  front  of  a  large  apiary  composed  of  particularly  strong  colonies  were  noted 
working  on  rhododendron  bloom  during  a  drizzling  rain  at  a  temperature  of 
60°  F.,  but  no  bees  were  noted  on  trees  or  from  weak  colonies  under  such 
conditions.  Water  collectors  appear  to  come  out  on  their  short  flight  even 
during  a  light  rain  and  during  intervals  between  rains,  when  other  bees  remain 
in  the  hive,  and  this  has  been  observed  to  be  a  factor  in  poisoning,  such  bees 
gathering  poisoned  water  from  leaves  on  sprayed  trees  or  from  herbage  growing 
in  sprayed  orchards. 

The  numbers  of  bees  taken  at  the  different  relative  humidities  experienced 
during  the  course  of  these  investigations  have  been  tabulated  and  studied  with- 
out our  being  able  to  detect  any  direct  effect  of  relative  humidity  on  activity 
of  either  hive  or  wild  bees,  up  to  the  point  of  actual  precipitation. 

(e)  INFLUENCE  OF  TIME  OF  DAY 

The  curve  of  activity  throughout  the  day  is  of  interest  because  it  represents 
the  combined  effects  of  temperature,  light,  and  possibly  other  factors  such  as 


__80_3. 


__70_2 


■KE-Y" 


///re  Sees 
M/af    - 
_  7e/7?pera/£/rb  /="_ 


—  //um/a///y . 


— 3P- 


//our  o/  flat/ 


%-  J>600- 6.30-  700-   730-  d&O-  850-  900-   950-  10.00-  1130-  II 00-  11.30-  12  00-  1230-  1.00-   130-  2  00-  2.50"  300-  330-  4.00-  43(1-  5.00-  530- 
^  ^630  700  730    fiOO    ft30    900    930    10.00    1030    MOO   11.30    12.00  1230    100     1.30    2.00   230    500   330  400    430    5.00    550    600 

If 

IN 

Fig.  54. — Average  bee  activity  at  different  diurnal   periods  in  connection   with   humidity 
and  temperature,  1929-32    (original). 

the  possible  exhaustion  of  nectar  and  pollen  supplies.  As  indicated  in  the  fore- 
going chart  (smoothed  by  the  method  of  moving  averages),  it  will  be  seen  that 
this  follows  a  fairly  regular  curve  reaching  a  maximum  for  hive  bees  at  12-12.30 
p.m.  and  for  solitary  bees  at  11-11.30  a.m.  Temperature  and  humidity  have 
been  shown  for  comparison. 


119 

TABLE  NO.  17.— TABLE  SHOWING  AVERAGE  BEE  ACTIVITY  AT  DIFFERENT  DIURN\L 
PERIODS  IN  CONNECTION  WITH  TEMPERATURE  AND  HUMIDITY* 


HiV€ 

bees 

Wild  bees 

Temperature 

Humidity 

Time 

Average 
number 

Number 
of  obser- 
vations 

Average 
number 

Number 
of  obser- 
vations 

Average 

Number 
of  obser- 
vations 

Average 

Number 
of  obser- 
vations 

6  00-6  30 

0 

0-4 

0-23 

0-55 

1-29 

2-34 

2-27 

2-35 

2-70 

2-69 

2-47 

3-46 

300 

1-43 

3-01 

2-27 

1-84 

1-95 

1-42 

1-95 

1-94 

1-89 

1-65 

1-67 

0 

16 

15 

22 

11 

102 

163 

227 

218 

252 

220 

244 

161 

10 

23 

213 

255 

279 

237 

293 

212 

223 

151 

96 

6 

2 

0 

0 

005 

009 

0-40 

106 

1-37 

1-50 

1-84 

1-47 

1-61 

1-58 

4-60 

1-91 

161 

1-47 

1-30 

1-56 

1-20 

113 

0-93 

0-74 

0-60 

0 

0 

16 

15 

22 

11 

102 

163 

227 

218 

252 

220 

244 

161 

10 

23 

213 

255 

279 

237 

293 

212 

223 

151 

96 

6 

2 

54-5 

61-75 

60-2 

61-0 

59-24 

59-81 

63-96 

62-73 

65-44 

64-28 

66-31 

66-27 

63-38 

72-3 

68-56 

67-74 

68-13 

67-20 

67-38 

66-83 

65  02 

63-63 

65-79 

60-67 

73-0 

4 

4 

9 

1 

37 

3  J 

52 

45 

59 

46 

58 

33 

8 

10 

57 

54 

76 

56 

77 

53 

64 

41 

24 

3 

1 

6o-5 

69-0 

62-0 

550 

61-24 

62-08 

59-0 

60-3 

59-96 

62-08 

60-76 

63-81 

61-14 

51-5 

54-98 

59-67 

58-30 

58-16 

57-58 

58-65 

58-72 

62  12 

60-84 

67-0 

420 

2 

6  30-7  00... 

3 

7  00-7.30 

7 

7  30-8.00 

2 

8  00-8.30 

34 

8  30-9.00 

26 

9  00-9.30 

45 

9  30-10.00 

40 

10.00-10.30 

10.30-11.00 

11.00-11.30 

11.30-12.00 

12.00-12.30 

12  30-1.00 

50 
40 
49 
32 

7 
4 

1  00-1.30 

46 

1  30-2  00 

45 

2  00-2  30 

66 

2.30-3.00 

51 

3  00-3.30 

67 

3  30-4.00 

49 

4  00-4.30 

61 

4  30-5  00... 

42 

5.00-5.30 

25 

5.30-6.00 

3 

6.00-6.30 

1 

*Actual  figures. 

Supplementary  data  on  the  influence  of  the  various  climatic  factors  are  pre- 
sented in  the  following  pages  by  Mr.  J.  M.  Cameron,  following  which  a  sum- 
mary of  the  general  effect  of  these  factors  is  given. 


(/)  SUPPLEMENTARY  DATA  ON  CLIMATIC  FACTORS 

(J.  M.  Cameron) 

(i)  General. — Desiring  to  obtain  further  information  with  regard  to  the 
effect  of  climatic  factors  on  bees  at  periods  other  than  apple  bloom,  further 
data  were  secured  in  August  and  September  during  the  period  of  maximum 
golden-rod  bloom,  many  other  flowers  such  as  fall  dandelion,  Canada  thistle, 
fireweed,  buckwheat,  white  clover,  etc.,  being  also  available.  Instead  of  attempt- 
ing to  count  visitors  to  the  flowers,  however,  as  was  done  during  apple  bloom  (1) 
the  bees  were  trapped  for  short  periods  after  the  manner  described  by  Farrar 
(1931);  (2)  counts  were  made  using  a  photoelectric  apparatus;  (3)  a  tripping 
device  was  used  to  register  and  record  automatically  the  insects  leaving  the 
hive.  By  following  the  latter  two  methods  we  did  not,  of  course,  secure  counts 
from  the  entire  hive,  but  only  for  a  small  area  in  the  centre  of  the  brood  chamber. 
It  would  have  been  preferable  to  have  securrd  counts  for  the  entire  colony, 
but,  as  the  necessary  apparatus  was  not  available,  it  wras  considered  that  the 
figures  obtained  in  this  way  would  indicate  the  comparative  activity  through- 
out the  day,  as  influenced  by  various  climatic  factors.  After  experimenting 
with  the  other  methods,  the  photoelectric  device  was  retained  as  giving  the  most 
satisfactory  and  accurate  results. 

Mr.  J.  S.  Leefe  assisted  in  taking  all  the  records  and  in  compiling  the  data 
obtained. 

(ii)  Apparatus  and  Its  Manipulation. — Figures  1,  2  and  3  show  the  essen- 
tial details  of  the  counting  device.     Figure  1  is  a  diagram  of  the  wiring  system 


120 

used.  There  are  four  different  circuits  in  the  hook-up.  The  primary  circuit  is 
that  of  the  photron  cell.  This  is  connected  with  a  Weston  Model  30  relay 
(fig.  1,  C).  As  the  bee  passes  through  the  tunnel  (fig.  2,  K)  it  interrupts  the 
light  beam  passing  through  the  slits  Q  and  R  and  falls  on  the  cell  B.  This 
interruption  varies  the  potential  in  the  circuit  and  operates  relay  C.  Operation 
of  this  relay  causes  completion  of  the  secondary  circuit,  a  4^-volt  direct  current 
supplied  by  three  dry  cells  (fig.  1,  H).  This  current  in  turn  operates  a  second 
relay  (fig.  1,  D),  which  acts  as  a  switch  in  the  110-volt  alternating  current 
line,  and  operates  the  counter  (fig.  1,  E).  The  fourth  circuit  has  an  eight- volt, 
alternating  current  obtained  through  a  transformer  from  the  110-volt  line.  This 
supplies  the  power  to  a  32  candle-power  bulb,  such  as  is  used  in  the  ordinary 
automobile  headlight. 


Fig.  55. — Diagram  of  photo-electric  bee  counter.  (A)  8-volt,  32-candle  power  bulb;  (B) 
Weston  photron  cell;  (C)  Weston  model  30  relay;  (D)  secondary  relay;  (E)  elec- 
trical counter;  (F)  transformer;  (G)  knife  switch;  (H)  battery  of  three  dry  cells: 
(I)  cone-shaped  lamp  box;  (J)  Abbe  condenser;  (K)  tunnel  (exit)  ;  (L)  tunnel 
(entrance);  (M)  landing  board;  (N)  bracket  supporting  light;  (0)  adjustable  bracket 
for  light  socket;  (P)  concave  mirror;  (Q)  slit  in  roof  of  tunnel;  (R)  cover  slip  over 
hole  in  floor  of  tunnel;    (S)   celluloid  trap  door  at  mouth  of  tunnel    (original). 


Figures  2  and  3  show  the  essential  details  of  the  hive  unit.  Figure  3  is  a 
front  view,  and  figure  2,  a  cross-section.  The  landing  board  is  cut  off  flush  with 
the  inner  face  of  the  front  of  the  hive,  and  the  unit  substituted.  The  entrance 
is  divided  into  a  number  of  tunnels  about  three-eighths  of  an  inch  square,  and 
approximately  three  inches  long.  These  are  closed  at  alternate  ends  by  a 
trap-door,  so  that  passage  is  possible  in  only  one  direction.  The  material  used  for 
the  trap  was  ordinary  photographic  film  hung  on  two  narrow  hinges  of  cello- 
phane. These  hinges  had  to  be  renewed  quite  frequently,  as  the  bees  often 
gnawed  them  off.    The  light  was  enclosed  in  a  cone-shaped  guard  directly  over 


121 

a  slit  through  one  of  the  exit  tunnels,  and  the  centre  of  the  photoelectric  cell 
was  placed  immediately  beneath.  This  arrangement  did  not  provide  a  suffi- 
ciently strong  light  to  actuate  the  cell,  so  an  Abbe  condenser  from  a  compound 
microscope  was  inserted  in  the  beam  to  concentrate  it.  This  made  a  great 
improvement,  and  the  addition  of  a  concave  mirror  above  the  bulb  gave  a 
sufficiently  strong  light  on  the  cell  to  cause  very  rapid  reactions.  The  instru- 
ment as  built  was  quite  crude,  and  by  adding  refinements,  such  as  the  use  of  a 
point-light  bulb,  could  be  greatly  improved. 

To  operate  the  instrument,  the  hive  unit  was  placed  on  the  front  of  the 
hive,  making  it  necessary  for  the  bees  to  enter  and  leave  through  the  tunnels. 
The  counter  was  then  plugged  into  the  110-volt  current,  causing  the  light  to 
come  on,  and  the  knife-switch  (fig.  1,  G)  was  closed.  This  prepared  all  the 
circuits,  and  each  time  a  bee  passed  out  through  the  tunnel,  interrupting  the 
light  beam,  it  was  recorded  by  the  counter.  Records  were  taken  every  quarter 
hour,  and  the  counter  set  back  to  zero. 

Light  intensity  records  were  taken  by  means  of  a  photoelectric  solar  radia- 
tion instrument  already  described.  Wind  velocities  were  obtained  by  the  use 
of  a  cup  type  anemometer,  and  temperatures  and  humidities  by  thermographs 
and  hygrographs  respectively. 

(iii)  Temperature. — Exits  were  recorded  at  the  lowest  temperature  obtained, 
viz.,  52°  F.  While  fairly  high  counts  were  obtained  between  this  and  60°  F.,  it 
is  believed  that  these  represent  mainly  "  play  flights/'  as  the  bees,  in  many 
cases,  did  not  leave  the  immediate  vicinity  of  the  hive.  Little  increase  in  flight 
activity  was  observed  until  a  temperature  of  62°  F.  was  reached.  From  this 
point  until  the  highest  recorded  temperature,  viz.,  84°  F.,  was  reached,  flight 
activity  showed  a  steady  and  almost  uniform  increase. 


Fig.  56. — Bee  activity  in  relation  to  temperature  during  golden-rod  honey  flow    (original). 

That  the  effect  of  temperature  is  modified  by  other  factors  is  clearly 
brought  out  by  the  fact  that  activity  is  higher  at  practically  all  recorded  tem- 
peratures in  the  morning  than  in  the  afternoon.  Few  "  work  flights  "  occurred 
in  the  afternoon  below  68°  F.,  and  from  this  temperature  to  58°  F.  activity 
remained  practically  constant  and  consisted  mainly  of  "  play  flights."  From 
58°  F.  to  56°  F.  recorded  flights  dropped  rapidly  to  zero  (fig.  60). 

(iv)  Light. — Within  the  range  of  conditions  permitting  free  flight,  light  is 
the  most  important  factor  influencing  activity.  It  is  very  noticeable  that  if,  on 
a  bright  day,  a  cloud  suddenly  obscures  the  sun,  outgoing  flight  immediately 
drops,  while  the  bees  wThich  are  out  return  in  large  numbers  almost  at  once. 
This  observation  was  made  on  several  occasions,  but  unfortunately  we  had  no 
means  of  measuring  such  inward  flight. 


122 


J— 


T 


^ 


\\l 


h 


> 


e        s        s ? g — 


tt 


22* 


if         * 


«       * 


r'Z 


S3 


ii    i 

«S       c 
£3      fa 


Me, 


ii 
si 

4«> 


33 


IS      - 


123 

Flight  after  about  2.30  p.m.  drops  off  with  increasing  rapidity.  Our  counts 
obtained  after  about  4  to  5  p.m.  cannot  be  taken  as  indicative  of  the  actual 
activity  of  the  colony,  as  observation  seemed  to  indicate  that  the  flights  taking 
place  at  this  time  were  largely  what  are  known  as  "  play  flights."  For  the 
same  reason  early  morning  flights,  up  until  about  eight  o'clock,  cannot  be  taken 
as  indicating  working  activity.  From  this  standpoint  the  method  of  counting 
bees  visiting  blossoms,  as  was  done  during  apple  bloom,  is  a  more  accurate 
criterion  of  field  activity  than  counting  the  bees  as  they  leave  the  hive. 

The  curve  of  average  flight  throughout  the  day,  as  shown  in  the  chart,  fol- 
lows more  closely  the  changes  in  light  values,  and  particularly  the  ultra-violet, 
than  those  in  temperature.  This  fact  was  borne  out  by  calculating  simple  cor- 
relations of  the  average  temperature,  intensity  of  clear  light  and  intensity  of  ultra- 
violet light,  with  number  of  bees,  at  different  periods  throughout  the  day.  The 
correlations  were  found  to  be:  for  temperature,  0-6686±  0-0520;  for  intensity 
of  clear  light,  0-8429±0-0305;  and  for  intensity  of  ultra-violet  light,  0-9156dt 
0-0141.  From  these  results  it  would  appear  that  ultra-violet  light  intensity  is 
the  most  important  factor  influencing  the  trend  of  flight  throughout  the  day. 
That  all  three  conditions  acting  together  probably  decide  the  content  of  activity 
is  shown  by  the  multiple  correlation  of  0-9332  which  was  found  to  exist. 

The  importance  of  ultra-violet  light  is  further  shown  by  the  beta-values 
calculated.  These  were,  for  ultra-violet  light,  clear  light  and  temperature, 
1-2972,  — -1709  and  —-2584,  respectively.  Of  these,  only  the  value  for  ultra- 
violet is  significant.  The  other  values  are  negative  and  unimportant.  This 
indicates  that  the  high,  positive,  simple  correlation  obtained  for  these  two  factors 
was  due  to  their  being  so  closely  associated  with  ultra-violet. 

There  is  considerable  variation  in  individual  observations,  and  these  do 
not  show  as  close  correlation  as  is  found  in  the  averages.  Individual  counts  seem 
to  be  somewhat  more  dependent  on  temperature  than  on  light,  the  simple  cor- 
relations of  temperature,  clear  light  and  ultra-violet  light  with  activity  being 
0-6179  ±  -0203,  0-5003  ±  -0245,  and  0-5346  ±  -0263,  respectively.  The 
multiple  correlation  of  these  factors,  using  the  415  observations  made,  was 
found  to  be  0-6517.  The  beta-values  in  this  case  show  temperature  to  be  the 
most  important  factor.  Ultra-violet  light  is  also  a  highly  significant  factor, 
while  clear  light  has  a  significant  bearing  on  activity,  but  is  not  as  important 
as  are  the  other  factors  mentioned. 

Taken  at  their  face  value,  the  above  results  indicate  that  the  average  trend 
of  flight  throughout  the  day  is  more  dependent  on  the  intensity  of  ultra-violet 
light  than  on  the  other  factors  studied;  while  any  individual  count  of  flight  is 
influenced  to  a  greater  degree  by  temperature.  This  last  condition  is  probably 
due  to  the  circumstance  that  there  is  a  threshold  temperature,  below  which 
flight  will  not  take  place  no  matter  how  favorable  the  light  conditions,  while 
above  this  temperature,  even  with  the  light  intensities  registering  practically 
nil,  there  is  nearly  always  some  flight.  The  relative  unimportance  of  the  clear 
light  factor  is  hard  to  understand. 

In  order  to  determine  further  the  importance  of  light  in  influencing  bee 
activity,  records  were  taken  during  the  partial  (93%)  solar  eclipse  of  August 
31,  1932.  Conditions  for  observation  at  this  time  were  practically  perfect. 
After  the  first  few  observations  there  was  a  dead  calm.  With  the  exception  of 
one  period  of  about  ten  minutes  no  clouds  interfered  with  the  light  conditions 
and  the  maximum  drop  in  temperature  was  seven  degrees  F.  A  glance  at  the 
accompanying  graph  will  show  the  close  relation  between  the  light  intensities 
and  the  bee  activity.  While  the  lowest  bee  count  was  obtained  at  the  period 
of  lowest  temperature,  the  fact  that  temperature  was  not  of  prime  importance 
is  shown  when  it  is  remembered  that  the  counts  obtained,  viz.,  eight  bees,  was 
only  a  mere  fraction  of  the  average  count  obtained  at  this  temperature. 


124 


500  510 

Fig.  58. — Distribution  of  green,  red  and  blue  during  partial  solar  eclipse,  August  31,   1932 

(original) . 

The  rise  in  activity  as  the  sun  again  came  into  view  was  not  as  regular  as 
the  decrease,  partly  due  to  the  fact  that  it  was  quite  late  in  the  afternoon,  when 
flight  normally  falls  off  fairly  rapidly  and  tends  to  become  more  irregular. 


310  3?0  330  340  350  40  410  420'  430  440  450  5M  311 

Fig.  59/ — Solar  radiation   in  relation  to   bee   activity  during  partial   solar   eclipse, 

August  31,  1932   (original). 

(v)  Wind. — No  wind  velocities  exceeding  4-4  miles  per  hour  were  obtained 
and  owing  to  varying  light  and  temperature  conditions  it  is  impossible  to  draw 
definite  conclusions  as  to  the  effect  of  this  factor  on  activity. 

(vi)  Humidity. — A  study  of  the  data  revealed  little  direct  connection  between 
bee  activity,  as  measured  in  flights  from  the  hive,  and  varying  degrees  of  relative 
humidity.     While  it  has  generally  been  stated  that  actual   precipitation  com- 


125 

pletely  stops  all  flight,  on  numerous  occasions  flight  was  noted  during  light  rains. 
This  activity  represented  mainly  "  play  flights  "  and  flights  of  water  carriers. 

(vii)  Activity  Throughout  the  Day. — The  combined  effects  of  the  different 
factors  already  discussed  are  indicated  in  the  trend  of  flight  activity  throughout 
the  day,  as  will  be  seen  by  a  glance  at  fig.  57.  The  figures  for  periods  earlier 
than  8.30  a.m.  have  only  limited  value,  since  they  represent  the  records  of  two 
days  only. 


M 

^^ 

IXIf 

,-. 

^/ 

/, 

^ 

25fi 

/ 

-'-^ 

?iwi 

/ 

/ 

\        / 

/ 

ion 

V 

/ 

H 

' 

<?e/rb.o 

/ '  />ees  a 

fab/fere 

'/?/ '  fe/rrpt 

"raii/res 

0 

/ 

after/poor, 

J 

r«* 

4    55    5 

6     51    5 

8     53     t 

0     61     e 

2     63     6 

4    65     6 

7emp 

b     67     6 
erafure 

8    69     7 

r. 

1      71       7 

Z      73      7 

\      75      7 

>     77      78     79  .80     Ml      82     83    84 

J'      1         1:        i 

Fig.  60. — Comparison  of  flight  of  bees  in  morning  and  afternoon    (original) 


6.   NUMBER  OF   VISITS   OF   HIVE  AND   WILD   BEES 

There  is  a  considerable  amount  of  scattered  observation  available  as  to 
the  number  of  trips  made  by  bees  and  the  number  of  individual  visJts  to  blossoms 
made  in  a  given  time.  Most  of  these  observations  relate  to  hive  bees.  It  seems 
very  difficult,  however,  to  arrive  at  any  average  figure  on  the  basis  of  these  data 
or  on  those  secured  during  the  course  of  our  own  observations. 

Lundie  (1925)  has  observed  that  the  morning  and  evening  flights  are  very 
much  shorter  than  those  that  take  place  in  the  main  period  of  the  day's  flight,  a 
variation  of  from  15  minutes  to  as  much  as  1  hour  and  43  minutes  occurring  in 
one  day.  Taking  all  the  days  for  which  the  average  duration  of  the  trips  was 
determined,  it  is  found  that  this  duration  varies  from  8  minutes  to  as  much  as 
1  hour  and  54  minutes.  In  the  honey  flow  the  trips  are  much  shorter  than  they 
are  in  a  dearth.  On  the  day  on  which  maximum  flight  occurred  it  was  found  that 
20-74  trips  per  bee  was  the  largest  possible  number  that  could  be  made  provided 
each  bee  entered  and  left  the  hive  immediately ;  while  the  figures  available  seemed 
to  show  that  even  in  a  heavy  honey  flow  the  bees  spent  more  time  in  the  hive 
between  trips  than  they  did  on  the  trip  itself. 

Parks  (1928)  found  that  one  hour  was  ample  time  for  a  nectar  carrier,  and, 
under  favourable  conditions,  ten  trips  a  day  was  considered  a  fair  average. 
Round  trips  for  pollen  may  be  made  in  15  minutes  or  less,  but  no  conclusion  is 
reached  as  to  the  number  of  trips  per  day.  Water  carriers  can  make  a  round 
trip  in  5  minutes  and  may  make  100  trips  per  day. 

Dyce  (1927)  states  that  the  average  number  of  blossoms  visited  by  a  colony 
of  bees  in  a  single  day  is  around  21,600,000  or  the  total  number  of  blossoms  on 


126 

20  acres  if  all  are  in  bloom  at  once.  A  low  estimate  of  the  average  number  of 
blossoms  visited  by  a  single  bee  in  a  day  is  about  720.  McCulloch  (1914) 
records  observations  in  which  one  bee  was  observed  visiting  61  blossoms,  another 
53  and  several  25  to  40,  and  many  similar  observations  have  been  made  by  us. 

If  a  pollen  carrier  made  only  ten  trips  a  day,  which,  on  the  basis  of  Park's 
figures  would  mean  2\  hours  in  the  field,  it  would  only  have  to  visit  72  blossoms 
on  each  trip  in  order  to  attain  the  figure  mentioned  by  Dyce  (loc.  cit.).  The 
figures  given,  however,  would  seem  to  indicate  a  field  force  of  30,000  bees  which 
is  considerably  stronger  than  the  average  colonies  obtained  by  us  at  this  time 
of  year.  Moreover,  allowance  would  again  have  to  be  made  for  the  proportion 
of  bees  not  carrying  pollen. 

On  the  other  hand,  not  all,  and  probably  not  more  than  half  the  blossoms 
on  mixed  plantings  are  available  in  the  proper  condition  for  the  bees  at  any  one 
time.  If  we  reduce  the  field  force  indicated  above  by  one  half  and  consider 
that  only  25  per  cent  carry  pollen,  this  would  give  us  an  effective  force  of 
3,750  per  colony.  If  each  of  these  pollinated  a  total  of  720  blossoms  throughout 
the  entire  bloom,  it  would  mean  the  pollination  of  2,700,000  blossoms  or  the 
total  blossoms  on  2-7  acres.  Provided  that  an  adequate  supply  of  effective 
pollen  is  available,  and  even  allowing  for  the  maximum  number  of  duplicate 
visits,  it  would  seem,  on  the  most  conservative  estimate,  that  the  force  liberated 
by  a  single  colony  of  bees,  should  be  capable  of  visiting  a  sufficient  proportion 
of  the  bloom  to  pollinate  effectively  several  acres  of  orchard,  the  exact  amount 
depending  upon  the  duration  of  favourable  flight  conditions.  It  should  be  noted 
that  only  a  small  proportion  of  the  bloom  needs  to  be  pollinated,  which  gives  a 
wide  margin  for  failures  to  be  visited,  for  ineffective  pollinations  and  for  duplicate 
visits. 

When  we  come  to  the  question  of  bumble  bees,  still  less  information  is  avail- 
able; though  a  number  of  workers  have  noted  that  these  work  more  rapidly  and 
cover  more  blossoms  in  a  given  time  than  do  hive  bees,  a  fact  which  our  own 
observations  substantiate.  Owing  to  the  small  size  and  rapid  movements  of 
our  chief  pollinator,  Halictus  smilacince  Robt.,  however,  it  was  not  possible  to 
secure  any  data  of  sufficient  accuracy  to  be  worthy  of  record.  Hive  bees  and 
bumble  bees  will  sometimes  be  observed  following  a  limb  and  covering  a  large 
percentage  of  the  blossoms  before  passing  on  to  another,  whereas  the  solitary 
bees  seem  to  be  less  consistent  in  their  movements.  Though  we  have  no  reliable 
data  as  to  the  mixtures  of  blossoms  individual  solitary  bees  may  visit  in  a 
given  period,  the  observations  made  in  another  section,  with  respect  to  the 
actual  number  present  in  the  bloom,  gives  useful  information  as  to  the  relative 
value  of  hive  and  solitary  bees. 

7.  POLLEN  GATHERING  HABITS 

Certain  pollen  gathering  habits  may  affect  the  evaluation  of  hive  and  wild 
bees  as  orchard  pollinators.  The  division  of  labour  among  worker  bees  into 
pollen  carriers,  nectar  carriers,  etc.,  means  that  the  force  available  for  pollina- 
tion is  reduced  in  proportion  to  the  numbers  devoted  to  their  special  tasks. 
Nectar  carriers  may  carry  little  pollen  and  certainly  have  less  value  for  this 
purpose  than  pollen  gatherers.  They  have  been  observed  on  many  visit-  to 
insert  their  mouth  parts  into  the  nectaries  from  the  side,  without  covering  the 
blossom  in  such  a  way  as  to  insure  pollination.  The  relative  number  of  bees 
which  collect  pollen  and  those  which  collect  nectar  is.  therefore,  of  considerable 
importance  in  connection  with  this  study.  Some  plants  are  visited  entirely 
for  pollen,  some  for  nectar  and  some  for  both  pollen  and  nectar.  According  to 
Parker  (1926)  when  a  bee  is  collecting  from  a  purely  pollen  plant  it  carries 
honey  to  moisten  the  pollen  and,  when  gathering  from  a  plant  that  produces 
both  nectar  and  pollen  it  invariably  obtains  nectar  from  the  plant  with  which 


127 

to  moisten  the  pollen.  In  our  work  it  was  found  that  on  some  days  collections 
from  apple  bloom  showed  a  comparatively  high  percentage  of  nectar  carriers 
and,  on  others,  a  much  lower  percentage.  Studies  in  nectar  secretion  showed 
that  during  some  days  in  which  there  was  a  certain  amount  of  activity  of  bees, 
the  nectaries  were  empty  or  nearly  so,  and  all  bees  present  were  gathering  pollen. 
It  was,  therefore,  very  difficult  to  arrive  at  any  average  percentage  of  pollen  and 
nectar  collectors  for  the  blossoming  period.  In  regard  to  this  problem  Simmins 
(1904)  states  as  follows: 

"  It  is  but  seldom  a  bee  gathers  a  large  load  of  both  pollen  and  honey  on 
one  and  the  same  journey.  A  pollen  gatherer  will  have  little  honey,  while 
those  carrying  the  most  honey  will  seldom  stay  for  a  particle  of  pollen,  more 
than  what  may  be  brushed  into  honey  as  collected.  The  pellets  are  brought  in 
most  freely  up  to  11  a.m.  while  everything  is  moist  from  the  dew  of  night;  or 
at  any  time,  immediately  after  a  shower,  if  warm.  The  honey  sources  of  the 
day  are  about  dried  up  by  three  p.m.  and  the  bees  do  not  often  work  actively 
after  that  time.     As  in  the  early  morning,  they  then  carry  in  much  water  to 


FlG.  61. — Some  pollinators  of  the  apple  showing  method  of  carrying  pollen.  (1)  Halictus 
smilacinae  Robt.;  (2)  Halictus  craterus  Lov.;  (3)  Halictus  coriaceous  Smith;  (4) 
Temnostroma  veuustum   Will. — Syrphidae —  (original ) . 


128 

help  in  preparing  the  food  for  the  young."  Filmer  (1932)  finds  great  irregu- 
larity in  the  proportional  number  of  pollen  carriers  during  fruit  bloom,  which 
bears  no  relation  to  the  total  field  force,  the  average  percentage  for  six  3-pound, 
overwintered  colonies  being  39-6;  for  four  3-pound  packages  established  on 
drawn  comb  24-7,  and  for  four  3-pound  packages  established  on  foundation 
15-7. 

Parker  (loc.cit.)  in  studies  of  a  number  of  selected  plants  records  a  count 
of  45  hive  bees  collecting  pollen  only,  24  collecting  both  pollen  and  nectar  and 


Fig.  62. — Hive  and  bumble  bees  showing  characteristic  methods  of  carrying  pollen:  i  I)  Apis 
mellifica  L..  showing  pollen  attached  to  hairs  of  body;  (2)  Apis  mellifica  L...  with  pollen 
baskets  full;  (3)  Bremus  temarius  Say.  showing  pollen  attached  to  hairs  of  body: 
(4)  Bremus  ferritins  Fab.,  with  pollen  baskets  full   (original). 


129 

31  collecting  nectar  only,  on  western  crab  apple.    On  apple  this  count  was  22-9 
and  69  respectively. 

During  the  period  of  apple  bloom  at  Ottawa  in  1932,  Mr.  A.  H.  W.  Birch, 
of  the  Division  of  Apiculture,  made  observations  on  approximately  7,000  bees 
entering  the  hives,  of  which  only  23-57  per  cent  were  pollen  gatherers.     Full 


,:\ 


VJ 


^v 


'fie  S^-'  -•-  @) 


_ 


^ 


3 


' 


, «,  . 


#•• 


to 


Fig.  63. — Pollen  mixtures  from  bees:  (1)  Apis  mellifica  L.  from 
Pyrus  malus  L.,  pollen  P.  mains  and  Taraxacum;  (2)  A. 
mellifica  L.  from  P.  malus  L.,  pollen  P.  malus  L.  and  Cara- 
gana;  (3)  A.  mellifica  L.  from  P.  malus  L.,  pollen  P.  malus  L. 
and  V actinium;  (4)  A.  mellifica  L.  from  Taraxacum,  pollen 
Taraxacum  and  Tulipa;  (5)  Br  emus  fervidus  Fab.  from  P. 
malus  L.,  pollen  P.  malus  L.  and  Daucus  Carota  L.;  (6) 
Br  emus  vagans  Smith  from  Garagana,  pollen  Caragana  and 
Tulipa;  (7)  A.  mellifica  L.  from  Taraxacum,  pollen  Taraxa- 
cum, Tulipa  and  V actinium;  (8)  A.  mellifica  L.  from  Spiraea, 
pollen  P.  malus  L.  and  Spiraea;  (9)  A.  mellifica  L.  from 
Taraxacum,  pollen  Taraxacum  and  Tulipa;  (10)  Awdrewa 
carlini  Ckll.  from  P.  malus  L.,  pollen  P.  malus  L.  and  !ZVt- 
folium   (original). 

details  of  this  work  are  given  elsewhere.  A  more  careful  examination  of  large 
numbers  of  nectar  gatherers  to  determine  what  proportion  of  them  are  of  value 
as  pollinators  for  apple  bloom  would  appear  to  be  indicated. 

It  would  not  appear  that  we  would  be  justified  in  considering  more  than  50 
per  cent  of  the  hive  bees  present  in  the  apple  bloom  as  effective  pollinators; 
whereas,  in  the  solitary  bees  where  this  division  of  labour  does  not  obtain,  it 
would  seem  that  all  females  captured  should  be  counted  as  potential  pollinators, 
solitary  bees  being  mainly  interested  in  pollen  to  prepare  the  pellets  for  their 

60796—9 


130 


, ,  ,  4p/5/??e///f/ca 

Fig.  64.— Pollen  collecting  apparatus  of  insect  pollinators  oi  the  apple    (original), 


131 

brood.  In  the  case  of  Andrena?,  a  certain  proportion  taken  were  males  which 
are  of  inferior  value  as  pollinators,  but,  since  this  proportion  is  small  and 
most  of  our  apple  visitors  were  Halicti,  this  would  be  of  little  importance. 
Though  more  erratic  in  their  flights,  wild  bees  work  the  bloom  with  thorough- 
ness and  persistence.  The  pollen  carrying  apparatus  of  the  hive  bee,  while 
more  efficient,  does  not  favour  this  species  in  comparison  with  solitary  bees  from 
the  standpoint  of  pollination.  With  the  latter  the  pollen  appears  to  be,  in 
general,  more  loosely  adherent  and  consequently  more  likely  to  be  brushed  off 
during  visits  to  the  bloom.  The  pollen  packed  in  the  baskets  of  the  hive  bee 
is  of  no  significance  in  pollination,  only  those  grains  held  by  the  dense  covering 
of  branched  hairs  on  all  parts  of  the  body  being  available.  As  previously 
indicated,  solitary  bees  do  not  appear  to  work  the  same  limb  or  tree  so  per- 
sistently, but  it  may  be  that  this  habit  results  in  more  cross-pollinations  than 
would  otherwise  be  the  case. 

8.    INFLUENCE    OF    NECTAR    SECRETION    AND    AVAILABILITY 

OF    POLLEN 

(a)  GENERAL 

The  availability  of  food  in  the  form  of  either  nectar  or  pollen  is  recognized 
as  having  a  profound  effect  upon  the  activity  of  bees,  exhaustion  of  supplies  being 
followed  by  a  slackening  off  in  flight.  In  the  case  of  certain  plants  which  secrete 
only  at  certain  times  of  the  day,  it  is  possible  to  note  a  close  correlation  between 
nectar  secretion  and  activity.  In  large  orchard  areas  there  would  rarely  be  a 
sufficient  supply  of  bees  to  exhaust  the  supply  of  nectar,  and  during  the  blossom- 
ing period,  in  weather  favourable  for  flight,  pollen  supplies  are  always  abundant. 
On  several  days  when  practically  no  nectar  was  available  a  considerable  number 
of  bees  were  noted  gathering  pollen.  Varietal  preferences  of  bees  based  on 
differences  in  nectar  secretion  and  production  of  odour  no  doubt  occur,  as  noted 
by  MacDaniels  (1931).  The  concentration  of  bees  on  varieties  at  optimum  con- 
ditions of  secretion  and  concentration  has  also  been  noted.  Other  varieties  are 
preferred  for  the  comparatively  large  quantities  of  free  pollen  available,  as  in 
Golden  Russet  or  Northern  Spy. 

Since  nectar  secretion  was  capable  of  measurement,  however,  careful  records 
were  taken  over  a  period  of  several  days  on  several  varieties  of  apples,  at  Kent- 
ville,  Blomidon  and  Scott's  bay,  N.S.,  to  determine,  if  possible,  the  factors 
influencing  nectar  secretion  and  the  influence  of  the  latter  on  bee  activity.  So 
far  as  hive  bees  are  concerned,  nectar  secretion  would  be  expected  to  influence 
mainly  the  nectar  gatherers,  but  it  was  considered  advisable  to  study  the  secre- 
tion of  nectar  in  relation  to  general  bee  activity  and,  particularly,  to  that  of 
solitary  bees.  Unfortunately,  we  had  no  facilities  for  determining  sugar  con- 
centration, which  may  well  have  an  important  bearing  on  attractiveness. 

The  records  regarding  nectar  secretion  were  taken  and  the  necessary  tabula- 
tions made  by  Mr.  Robert  Ward,  to  whom  the  writer  is  especially  indebted  for 
his  faithful  attention  to  the  details  of  the  work. 

(b)  RESULTS   AND   CONCLUSIONS 

A  complete  tabulation  of  all  the  data  secured  in  this  study  would  be  out  of 
place  at  this  point.  Neither  is  it  possible  to  draw  too  definite  conclusions  on  the 
basis  of  a  partial  season's  work.  Since  the  results  from  the  different  stations 
are  in  general  agreement  it  will  be  sufficient  for  our  purpose  to  consider  in  detail 
the  results  from  one  station  only  and  on  one  variety  only;  viz.,  Northern  Spy 
at  Scott's  bay. 

It  was  found  that  the  period  of  greatest  secretion  during  the  daylight  hours 
was  approximately  9  a.m.,  from  which  point  there  is  a  gradual  decrease  until 

60796— 9  i 


132 


late  in  the  afternoon.  It  would  appear  that  the  nectar  does  not  accumulate 
during  the  day,  but  ceases  after  a  definite  amount  has  been  deposited  in  the 
nectary,  the  processes  of  secretion  and  reabsorption  being  apparently  in  equilib- 
rium. 

The  results  obtained  from  a  study  of  secretion  during  the  night  varied  some- 
what, depending  upon  whether  successive  readings  were  made  from  different 
blossoms  or  from  repeated  records  from  all  the  blossoms  in  the  same  clusters, 
the  former  giving  the  maximum  results  at  midnight  and  the  latter  at  from  8.30  to 
10.50  p.m.  This  would  seem  to  indicate  that  nectar  accumulates  during  the 
night  up  to  a  certain  point,  which  may  be  determined  by  the  evaporation  rate. 
There  appears  to  be  a  positive  correlation  between  relative  humidity  and  nectar 
secretion,  but  in  some  cases  this  is  slight.  This  correlation  is  most  apparent  in 
the  night  readings. 

Bee  activity  is  so  contingent  upon  a  complex  of  factors  operating  simul- 
taneously, that  it  is  difficult  to  evaluate  any  one  of  them.  It  is  not  apparent  from 
the  data  secured  that  the  activity  of  either  wild  or  hive  bees  is  closely  con- 
nected with  nectar  secretion.  The  fact  that,  with  temperature  and  light  condi- 
tions favourable,  bees  show  greatest  activity  during  the  morning  hours  may  be 
governed  partly  by  the  greater  quantity  of  nectar  available  at  that  time.  Con- 
sidering the  fact  that  different  varieties  in  a  mixed  orchard  are  in  their  optimum 
condition  at  different  periods  and  considering  also  the  amount  of  nectar  avail- 
able during  apple  bloom  in  proportion  to  the  number  of  bees  present,  and  the 
importance  of  temperature,  light  and  wind  factors,  it  is  only  to  be  expected  that 
any  effect  due  to  variations  in  nectar  availability  might  be  effectively  disguised. 

9.  COMPARATIVE  CONSTANCY  OF  HIVE  AND  WILD   BEES 
The  following  important  advantage  has  been  claimed  for  the  hive  bee:  — 
That  the  hive  bees  show  greater  "flower  fidelity,"  that  is,  they  tend  to  visit 

only  one  species  of  flower  at  a  time  for  pollen  or  nectar,  whereas  ivild  species 

tend  to  visit  a  larger  number  of  species. 


Kt^L 


A?e/a,//i4f  //urn/*///? //? /&r £■&/?/ 


w 


7       8       9       ID        II       12      lam     Z      3       4       5      6       7 


/IhCTARyT-CRHIOATt-APf-RARIRh  AUAIDITY/WF-  &V1LD  Btt/ 


Fig.  65. — Bee  activity  iri  relation  to  nectar  secretion    (original) 


133 

Though  constancy  of  the  hive  bee  in  visiting  blossoms  of  a  single  species 
has  been  a  matter  of  comment  since  very  early  times,  our  figures  based  on  pollen 
loads  of  all  species  concerned  do  not  permit  us  to  conclude  with  any  degree  of 
certainty  that  any  of  the  species  studied  whether  hive  bees,  Halicti,  Andrense  or 
Bremi,  show  any  decided  advantage  over  the  other  in  regard  to  constancy  except 
that,  where  there  is  a  great  variety  of  species  in  bloom  without  any  one  pre- 
dominating, the  Andrenae  observed  showed  least.  Where  a  large  acreage  of 
apple  is  available,  all  forms  showed  a  considerable  degree  of  constancy,  exceeding 
in  all  cases  50  per  cent  and  often  reaching  100  per  cent,  but,  when  abundant 
bloom  of  many  different  species  was  available,  without  the  predominance  of  any 
one  plant  species,  the  constancy  exhibited  was  very  much  less.  Based  on  apple 
bloom  alone,  Apis  took  first  place  with  80  per  cent  of  pure  loads,  followed  by 
Halictus  with  72  per  cent,  Bremus  with  65  per  cent  and  Andrena  57  per  cent. 
Based  on  all  species  of  bloom,  Halictus  appeared  to  lead  Apis  in  constancy,  but 
the  difference  indicated  was  not  sufficiently  great  to  be  significant.  If  it  were 
possible  to  express  the  proportion  of  pollen  in  the  loads,  the  degree  of  constancy 
would  appear  greater  than  in  percentage  of  pure  and  mixed  loads,  since  a  large 
proportion  of  the  loads  classified  as  "  mixed  "  were  made  up  of  a  great  prepon- 
derance of  one  kind  of  pollen. 

10.  DISTRIBUTION  AND  CONCENTRATION 

As  indicated  in  other  sections  hive  bees  are  more  specialized  in  their 
habits  than  are  wild  bees,  and  this  affects  their  value  as  pollinators,  sometimes 
favourably,  sometimes  unfavourably.  It  has  been  shown  by  a  number  of 
investigators  that  hive  bees  possess  "  scouts  "  and  that  these  "  scouts "  are 
able  to  make  known  to  other  workers  the  presence  of  food  in  the  form  of  nectar 
or  pollen.  Hive  bees  have  been  repeatedly  observed  to  ignore  apple  bloom 
near  their  hive  to  seek  other  more  attractive  bloom  farther  afield  and,  on 
occasion,  to  concentrate  on  favoured  varieties  or  on  varieties  in  a  favourable 
condition  of  nectar  secretion  or  pollen  availability.  Wild  bees  are  not  con- 
sidered to  have  anything  in  the  nature  of  "  scouts  "  and  hence  would  not  be 
expected  to  have  the  same  tendency  to  concentrate  as  hive  bees,  though  they 
are  certainly  able  to  recognize  and  select  bloom  in  its  optimum  state  of 
attractiveness.  In  the  orchard,  however,  our  counts  are  more  consistent  with 
wild  bees,  even  where  colony  distribution  has  been  such  as  to  secure  maximum 
evenness  of  distribution  of  the  insects.  Sudden  concentrations  of  hive  bees, 
however,  examples  of  which  are  discussed  elsewhere,  have  occurred  on  a  num- 
ber of  occasions  in  the  course  of  our  observations.  Considerable  unevenness 
of  distribution  has,  however,  been  noted  with  solitary  bees,  the  exact  reason 
for  which  was  hard  to  determine. 

11.  GENERAL  SUMMARY 

(i)  The  great  present  scarcity  of  hive  bees  in  the  Annapolis  valley  accen- 
tuates the  importance  of  a  knowledge  of  the  native  fauna  concerned  in  apple 
pollination.  It  was  found  that  the  most  important  agents  at  the  present  time 
in  apple  pollination  were  various  species  of  solitary  bees.  These  bees  are 
found  everywhere  nesting  in  the  ground  in  roadside  banks,  pastures  sparsely 
covered  with  vegetation,  the  drier  parts  of  dyke  lands,  in  "  running  dykes  " 
and  similar  situations.  They  avoid  sandy  or  gravelly  soil  in  nesting  and, 
hence,  are  more  numerous  in  places  near  the  North  Mountain,  Long  island, 
etc.,  than  in  many  other  sections. 

(ii)  Observations  made  during  the  past  four  years  indicate  that  in  the 
average  orchard  the  number  of  solitary  bees  present  during  bloom  was  about 
equal  to  the  force  of  hive  bees  that  would  be  released  from  one  strong  colony 


134 

per  acre.  Nevertheless,  there  was  evidence  of  a  shortage  of  bees  in  certain 
individual  orchards,  and  during  1932  there  was  an  apparent  general  decline  of 
about  50  per  cent  in  the  population  of  solitary  bees.  It  should  be  noted  that 
the  three  years  previous  to  1932  were  exceptionally  favourable  for  the  increase 
of  the  solitary  bee  population. 

(iii)  Our  observations  indicate  that,  with  little  other  bloom  available,  one- 
third  of  the  total  field  force  of  hive  bees  may  be  found  at  one  time  in  the  apple 
blossoms.  Of  these  not  all  are  of  equal  value  in  pollination,  since  a  large  propor- 
tion are  nectar  gatherers.  Solitary  bees  visit  the  blossoms  mainly  for  the  purpose' 
of  collecting  pollen,  which  they  mix  with  a  little  nectar  and  use  for  brood  rearing 
purposes.  Therefore,  practically  the  entire  solitary  bee  population  would  be 
expected  to  be  potential  pollinators. 

(iv)  The  fact  that  hive  bees  only  can  be  artificially  increased  and  intro- 
duced at  will  into  the  orchards  is  a  decided  point  for  the  use  of  hive  bees.  No 
other  provision  is  possible  against  seasonal  fluctuations  in  the  normal  popula- 
tion of  solitary  bees. 

(v)  Hive  and  wild  bees  react  similarly  to  temperature,  light,  wind, 
humidity,  etc.;  but  hive  bees  appear  to  work  longer  hours,  and,  while  the 
optimum  temperature  for  both  is  about  the  same,  a  larger  proportion  of  hive 
bees  are  found  at  the  lower  temperature  ranges  and  lower  light  intensities. 
Furthermore,  the  fact  that  hive  bees  will  work  actively  near  the  hive  during 
brief  bursts  of  favourable  weather,  gives  an  advantage  to  orchards  so  supplied 
in  years  of  uncertain  and  changeable  weather  during  bloom. 

(vi)  Definite  figures  as  to  numbers  of  visits  of  wild  bees  are  not  available; 
but,  placing  the  lowest  possible  estimate  on  the  number  of  visits  made  by 
either  hive  or  wild  bees,  it  is  apparent  that  a  force  equal  to  that  liberated  from 
a  single,  strong,  overwintered  colony  would  be  theoretically  capable  of  effectively 
pollinating  a  number  of  blossoms,  representative  of  the  entire  complement  of 
an  acre  of  bearing  trees,  in  a  comparatively  short  period  of  favourable  weather. 

(vii)  In  pollen  gathering  habits  the  solitary  bees  are  equally  well  adapted 
for  pollination  purposes,  and,  in  some  respects,  appear  to  have  an  advantage. 

( viii)  Many  workers  have  claimed  a  superior  value  for  hive  bees  with  respect 
to  their  greater  constancy  to  the  flowers  visited,  but  we  were  not  able  to  confirm 
this  observation  in  our  studies,  the  difference  between  Halictus  and  Apis  not  being 
significant. 

(ix)  Owing  to  limitations  already  noted,  much  further  work  must  be  done 
before  many  of  the  points  raised  in  the  foregoing  pages  can  be  regarded  as 
satisfactorily  settled. 

E.  UTILIZATION  OF  HIVE  BEES  AS   ORCHARD   POLLINATORS 

W.  H.  Brittaix  and  C.  B.  Gooderham 

1.    HISTORICAL    REVIEW 

Many  workers  have  published  data  and  opinions  regarding  the  actual 
commercial  value  of  hive  bees  for  the  purpose  of  pollinating  apple  orchards.  A 
number  of  typical  examples  is  therefore  included,  in  order  to  give  a  represent- 
ative picture  of  the  present  status  of  opinion  regarding  this  matter. 

Gates  (1917)  states  that  bees  are  of  more  value  to  the  fruit-grower  than  to 
the  apiculturist,  because  of  their  work  in  pollinating  fruits.  Many  cases  are  cited 
in  which  absence  of  bees,  or  weather  which  prevented  their  working,  resulted  in 
poor  crops.  In  one  case,  a  high  south  wind  prevailed  during  the  blooming  period, 
and  as  a  result  the  north  sides  of  the  trees  alone  had  a  good  set.  as  the  bees 
worked  in  the  shelter. 

The  case  is  cited  by  Weed  (1918)  of  two  orchards  in  Wayne  county.  N.Y., 
kept  under  observation  at  blooming  time.    Both  had  fair  bloom— one  had  many 


135 

hive  bees  working  in  it,  the  other  practically  none.  The  crops  were  about  equal 
in  the  two  orchards.  Other  wild  bees,  as  Bombi,  Andrenidae,  Halictidae,  etc.,  were 
considered  to  be  responsible  for  the  pollination  of  the  orchard  which  had  no  hive 
bees  in  it. 

Sax  (1922)  states  that  good  crops  have  been  obtained  in  New  England, 
where  bumble  bees  were  apparently  the  only  pollinating  agents.  However,  honey 
bees  are  desirable  and  in  some  cases  indispensable. 

According  to  Haseman  (1922)  the  hive  bee  is  the  best  agent  for  the  pol- 
lination of  deciduous  fruit  trees.  Hive  bees  are  more  valuable  in  cool  weather 
than  at  any  other  time  because  the  supply  of  other  pollinating  insects  is  then  at 
its  lowest. 

Bercaw  (1924)  describes  experiments  sponsored  by  the  University  of  Cali- 
fornia at  the  instance  of  the  prune  growers,  showing  that  honey  bees  were  the 
chief  pollinators  of  deciduous  fruits  in  California.  Citrus  fruits  are  in  the  same 
class. 

DeOng  (1925)  emphasizes  the  danger  of  relying  upon  self-fertilization,  partic- 
ularly when  a  single  type  is  grown  in  large  areas;  and  states  that  under  most 
conditions,  dependence  must  be  placed  on  the  aid  of  insects,  and1  especially  the 
honey  bee,  for  pollination.  Bees  are  said  to  be  better  adapted  for  pollen  carrying 
than  other  insects,  but  some  are  of  greater  value  than  others.  He  notes  that 
bumble  bees,  carpenter  bees,  etc.,  are  not  active  early  in  the  spring,  or  if  so,  are  in 
such  small  numbers  that  they  are  comparatively  ineffective  as  pollinators,  and 
during  the  summer  they  have  only  one  to  six  brood  cycles;  whereas  the  honey 
bee  has  twelve  to  fifteen  broods  in  a  summer.  He  points  out  that  the  workers 
survive  the  winter  also,  while  all  except  the  queens  of  the  native  species  succumb. 
Another  advantage  of  hive  bees  noted  by  this  worker  is  that  their  numbers  can 
be  distributed  as  desired  in  the  orchards,  while  the  numbers  of  wild  bees  depend 
on  the  natural  surroundings. 

Because  bees  are  present  in  large  numbers  in  the  spring,  and  work  from 
morning  until  night  and  from  early  spring  until  late  fall,  they  are  regarded  as  the 
most  efficient  pollinators  of  deciduous  fruits,  by  Davis  (1926).  A  few  instances 
of  the  value  of  honey  bees  to  orchardists  are  given.  Two  hundred  hives  of  bees 
were  placed  at  one  end  of  an  80-acre  cherry  orchard  at  Belleview,  Ohio.  The  crop 
grew  lighter  in  proportion  to  the  distance  from  the  hives.  In  another  orchard 
the  bees  were  all  killed  by  foul  brood,  except  one  colony  which  was  placed 
in  the  centre  of  the  orchard.  There  was  a  definite  fruit  area  about  this  hive,  while 
the  other  trees  had  no  crop.  Orchardists  are  advised  to  have  one  colony  for  every 
fifty  trees,  and  to  avoid  spraying  at  blooming  time. 

The  importance  of  bees  to  the  fruit-grower  is  emphasized  by  Hendrickson 
(1927)  and  the  causes  of  decreased  numbers  of  wild  and  tame  bees  are  ascribed 
to  intensive  cultivation  and  lack  of  skill  in  beekeeping  respectively  on  the  part 
of  fruit-growers.  With  regard  to  the  distribution  of  bees  in  orchards,  a  case  is 
cited  in  which  fifteen  colonies  were  placed  on  each  side  of  a  thirty-acre  prune 
orchard.  A  heavy  crop  was  borne  along  the  edges,  but  in  the  centre  the  crop  was 
very  light. 

According  to  Barclay  (1928)  the  orchard  districts  in  New  Jersey  are  sur- 
rounded by  an  extensive  trucking  area,  and  as  a  result  wild  bees  of  all  kinds  are 
scarce.  He  states  that,  since  1918,  the  use  of  bees  for  pollinating  purposes  has 
increased  steadily.  One  hundred  colonies  were  rented  in  1918;  1,600  were  rented  in 
1927.  Prices  ranged  from  $5  to  $8  per  colony.  Blueberry  and  cranberry  growers 
are  faced  with  a  problem  similar  to  that  confronting  the  orchardists. 

Marshall,  Johnson,  Hootman,  and  Wells  (1929)  present  evidence  which  is 
considered  to  indicate  the  value  of  bees  in  orchard  pollination.  An  11-acre  Spy 
orchard  had  never  produced  more  than  1,500  bushels  in  any  season  from  1918  to 
1926,  even  though  it  contained  an  apiary  of  40  colonies.  In  1927  pollinizing 
bouquets  were  distributed  throughout  the  orchard,  which,  in  that  vear  produced 


136 

a  crop  of  5,200  bushels.  Other  examples  are  given.  The  commercial  fruit  grower 
is  said  to  be  almost  entirely  dependent  upon  the  hive  bee  to  insure  pollination 
of  his  fruit,  and  it  is  stated  that  there  are  not  enough  bees  in  many  orchards  at 
blossoming  time  to  insure  adequate  cross-pollination  during  certain  seasons  when 
the  weather  is  unfavourable  for  insect  activity.  It  is  claimed  that  small  orchards 
and  those  adjacent  to  uncultivated  fields,  woods  or  swamps,  where  wild  bees 
can  winter  in  satisfactory  numbers,  may  produce  crops  without  the  addition 
of  bees,  but  in  most  commercial  orchards  the  chances  of  a  good  crop  are  increased 
by  the  addition  of  one  colony  per  acre  in  mature  orchards,  or  one  to  four  acres 
in  orchards  10  to  15  years  old. 

Lundie  (1927)  states  that  out  of  the  119  varieties  of  apple  commonly  grown 
in  South  Africa,  77  are  self -sterile  and  the  others  give  bigger  crops  when  cross- 
pollinated.  He  points  out  that  honey  bees  in  South  Africa  are  able  to  rear  several 
brood  cycles  before  the  fruit  bloom  appears,  and  for  this  reason  are  the  most 
valuable  pollinating  insects.  He  regards  wild  swarms  as  having  some  value,  but 
not  comparable  to  that  of  tended  hives,  because  such  swarms  are  usually  weak  in 
the  spring.  He  contends  that  non-social  insects  are  not  numerous  enough  to  be  of 
importance  in  large  orchards,  but  do  some  good  work. 

In  a  popular  bulletin  by  Phillips  (1930)  the  place  of  the  honey  bee  in  the 
orchard  is  discussed.  Due  to  cultivation,  destruction  of  both  honey  bees  and  wild 
bees  by  dusting  and  spraying,  and  to  the  natural  scarcity  of  wild  bees  in  spring, 
honey  bees  are  considered  to  be  the  most  satisfactory  pollinating  agents. 

(a)  EXPERIMENTAL  USE  OF  HIVE  BEES 

During  the  entire  course  of  our  work,  poisoning  from  sprays  and  dusts  con- 
stituted a  disturbing  factor  and  rendered  abortive  much  of  the  work  attempted. 
This  and  other  causes  limited  the  scope  of  the  work.  Only  those  projects  that  we 
were  able  to  carry  to  completion  are  reported  herein. 

(i)  Tent  Experiments.  The  best  indication  of  the  necessity  for  bees  in 
order  to  ensure  proper  pollination  of  the  apple  is  obtained  from  a  study  of  the 
results  of  trees  enclosed  in  tents,  both  with  and  without  bees,  because  it  is  pos- 
sible to  control  the  conditions  of  the  experiment.  These  experiments  have  been 
discussed  in  detail  elsewhere,  but  the  following  tabulation  is  of  interest  in  con- 
nection with  the  present  discussion,  since  it  emphasizes,  for  all  varieties,  the 
necessity  of  bees  for  pollination,  as  well  as  the  primary  need  for  a  suitable  supply 
of  pollen  for  them  to  carry.  It  will  be  noted  that  even  such  a  highly  self- 
fruitful  variety  as  Baldwin  gives  improved  results  when  supplied  with  bees 
and  the  pollen  of  a  self-fruitful  variety.  The  "  bouquets  "  used  in  these  tests 
were  blossoming  limbs  of  the  desired  varieties  placed  in  tubs  of  water  inside 
the  tents;  bees  were  placed  in  the  orchard  to  provide  for  the  open  pollinated 
trees  and  abundant  sources  of  suitable  pollen  were  present. 

TABLE  No.  J8.— RESULTS  OF  TENT  EXPERIMENTS,  1929-1932 


Per  cent  fruit 

Variety 

Effective 
polli- 
nizer 

and  bees 

In- 
effective 
polli- 
nizer 
and  bees 

No 

polli- 

nizer 

and  bees 

(selfed) 

No 

polli- 

nizer 

and 

no  bees 

Effective 

polli- 

nizer 

but 

no  bees 

Open 
polli- 
nation 

Gravenstein 

10-90 
5-42 

817 
10  05 

114 
3-58 
4-96 
2-70 

212 

3-32 
7  •  77 
200 

0-67 
103 
3-49 
0-85 

0-47 
1-81 
5  05* 
1-20 

9-91 

4  74 

10-40 

Spy        

i  - 

*  Abnormally  high  percentage  fruit  obtained  in  1932,  due  to  small  number  of  blossoms  on  tree  used 
bas  raised  the  general  average  for  this  treatment. 


137 

The  velocity  of  the  wind  inside  and  outside  the  tent  was  taken  for  pur- 
poses of  record  and  because  of  its  bearing  on  wind  pollination  on  tented  trees. 
Considerable  irregularity  was  noted,  but  the  difference  between  tented  and  non- 
tented  trees  was  very  apparent.  In  order  to  equalize  results,  therefore,  a  draft 
from  an  orchard  duster  was  blown  through  the  bouquets  and  over  the  tree,  the 
velocity  being  30  miles  per  hour  at  six  feet  from  the  nozzle. 

(ii)  Field  Experiments. — To  secure  comparable  results  under  uncontrolled 
orchard  conditions  is  impossible,  because  fruitfulness  is  due  to  too  many  factors 
not  under  the  control  of  the  experimenter.  The  possibilities  of  selfing,  unfruit- 
ful crosses,  poisoning  of  bees  and  many  other  factors,  render  this  type  of  work 
very  uncertain  and  unsatisfactory.  Many  reports  of  spectacular  results  from 
the  use  of  bees  are  of  doubtful  value,  as  they  indicate  a  single  years'  observa- 
tions only  and  data  regarding  succeeding  crops  are  rarely  forthcoming.  Sudden 
increases  in  the  crop  of  individual  orchards  have  been  repeatedly  observed, 
under  conditions  sometimes  difficult  to  explain,  but  which  were  certainly  not 
due  to  this  particular  factor.  It  was  apparent  throughout  the  work  that  the 
presence  of  an  adequate  supply  of  effective  pollinizers  was  of  prime  importance 
and  that,  unless  a  fair  proportion  of  the  bloom  present  was  of  this  kind,  unfruit- 
ful crossing  or  selfing  negatived  the  results  of  bee  introduction. 

Where  such  effective  pollinizers  have  not  been  provided  when  the  orchard 
was  planted,  they  should  be  introduced  as  speedily  as  possible  by  top-working 
the  desired  number  of  trees.  The  temporary  expedient  of  pollinizing  "bouquets" 
consisting  of  limbs  of  the  desired  bloom  placed  in  tubs  throughout  the  orchard, 
has  been  recommended  by  a  number  of  workers,  but  there  are  several  disad- 
vantages attending  the  practice  even  as  a  temporary  measure.  A  great  deal  of 
labour  is  involved  in  securing  the  bouquets  and  in  keeping  them  fresh.  It  is 
very  difficult  to  procure  bloom  in  such  a  quantity  as  would  compare  with  the 
required  number  of  trees  in  full  bloom,  and  the  chances  of  selfing  or  unfruitful 
crossing  are  greater  than  for  fruitful  crosses.  Furthermore,  such  bouquets 
appear  to  be  much  less  attractive  than  full  trees.  Nevertheless,  in  experimental 
work  it  was  impossible  to  wait  for  "  grafting  out "  operations  and,  for  this  reason 
bouquets  were  employed. 

In  numerous  experiments  with  bouquets  we  were  never  able  to  secure  suffi- 
cient to  utilize  them  in  an  orchard  of  any  size  without  undue  cutting  of  the 
trees  used  as  the  source  of  the  bouquets.  Even  when  bouquets  were  obtained 
as  large  as  possible,  kept  fresh  and  placed  on  small  stands  surrounding  the 
trees,  we  could,  in  many  cases,  get  little  definite  evidence  of  value  from  the 
use  of  such  bouquets  in  orchards.  Furthermore,  counts  made  at  the  bouquets 
often  showed  few  bees  visiting  them  when  masses  of  other  bloom  were  available, 
though  bouquets  in  full  bloom,  obtained  from  a  later  district  and  placed  in 
orchards  just  out  of  bloom,  were  heavily  visited.  In  the  same  orchard  where 
negative  or  inconclusive  results  were  obtained  from  the  use  of  bouquets,  hand 
pollinations  with  the  same  variety  of  pollen  as  that  provided  by  the  bouquet 
gave  a  heavy  set  of  fruit.  Our  experiments  were  done  mainly  with  Blenheim, 
a  variety  in  which  pollination  difficulties  are  frequently  experienced.  By 
placing  rings  of  bouquets  around  a  group  of  trees,  or  by  enclosing  a  row  of  trees 
with  a  row  of  bouquets,  we  were  able,  in  one  case,  to  double  the  set  on  that  par- 
ticular row.  In  most  cases,  however,  the  results  were  not  significant  and  we 
were  unable  to  get  the  spectacular  results  obtained  by  certain  other  workers. 

The  result  of  only  one  such  experiment  under  the  optimum  conditions  for 
securing  such  results,  will  be  reported  in  detail.  The  experiment  was  conducted 
in  an  orchard  of  Blenheim,  Stark  and  Mann,  cross-unfruitful  and  self-unfruit- 
ful varieties,  consisting  of  17,  41  and  29  trees  respectively.  Hand  pollination 
tests  in  1931  indicated  that  a  pollination  problem  was  involved  and,  even  in 
that  year  when  Blenheim  everywhere  gave  a  crop,  this  orchard  failed  to  pro- 


138 

duce.  A  ring  of  bouquets  was  placed  about  the  trees  so  as  to  enclose  several 
of  each  variety.  Bouquets  of  cross-fruitful  varieties  were  used,  mainly  Wagener 
and  Red  Astrachan.  Owing  to  the  weather  there  was  no  difficulty  in  keeping 
the  bouquets  fresh;  they  were  visited  freely  by  bees  and,  though  wTeather  con- 
ditions during  the  bloom  were  unfavourable,  there  was  sufficient  time  during 
bloom  to  ensure  pollination,  since  50  hives  of  bees  were  placed  in  the  orchard. 


5 

3 

3 

3 

5 

JY 

5 

IV 

3 

S 

S" 

5 

5" 

S 

5" 

5 

5 

S 

5 

3" 

3 

3 

0 

3 

3 

3 

• 

- 

5 

5 

5 

5 

S 

S 

5 

5 

5 

5 

5 

3" 

5 

3l 

*  : 
3 

3 

3> 

S 

3" 

3 

5 

S 

S 

Sx 

5 

5 

S> 

> 

5 

0 

5 

5 

M 

Af 

Af 

Af 

<     X     ) 

Af* 

:    x 

Af 

Af 

S 

3 

Af" 

Af 

Af 

AT 

Af 

Af 

Af 

Af" 

5 

5 

Af 

Af 

AT 

AT 

Af 

Af 

Af 

AT 

5 

5 

3 

Af 

A! 

0 

Af 

Af 

0 

Af 

5 

LE-C 

3E-: 

3  - 

Af  - 
5  - 

/ID 

3/e/ 

Afa, 

5fcr, 

7/7 

77 

X     - 

»  -i 

Qeco 

rafA 

-ees 

FlG.  66. — Diagram  of  C.  Bishop  orchard. 
Somerset.  X.S.  (original). 

Blenheim  and  Mann  blossomed  well;  but  Stark,  which  bore  a  heavy  crop 
the  year  before,  had  little  bloom.  The  results  obtained  from  typical  trees  inside 
and  outside  the  ring  of  bouquets  are  indicated  in  the  accompanying  table.  They 
indicate  a  commercial  set  on  the  Blenheim  and  a  less  than  commercial  set  on 
Mann  which,  however,  were  in  a  particularly  poor  condition  from  a  nutritional 
standpoint.  The  fact  that  the  crop  decreased  as  the  distance  from  the  bouquets 
increased  was  plainly  evident. 

TABLE  No.  20 


Variety 

Position 

Number 

blossoms 

used 

Per  cent 
fruit 

Blenheim 

Within  bouquets 

Outside 

9,918 

10,000 

8,060 

8,077 

6-47 

« 

}•:;:; 

Mann 

Within         " 

2-30 

" 

Outside 

1-58 

According  to  the  owners  of  the  orchard,  the  crop  obtained  under  these  con- 
ditions on  Blenheim  which  had  a  full  bloom,  is  over  double  that  of  previous 
years.  Considering  the  large  amount  of  unfruitful  pollen  available  and  the 
comparatively  poor  nutritional  condition  of  the  orchard,  the  results  are  as  good  as 
could  be  expected.  Other  experiments  in  solid  blocks  of  Blenheim  gave  similar 
results  on  trees  immediately  adjacent  to  the  bouquets,  in  one  case  doubling 
it  as  compared  with  the  general  average  for  the  orchard. 


139 


140 
(6)  WHEN    HIVE    BEES    SHOULD    BE    USED 

It  must  be  admitted  that,  whereas  the  opinions  of  most  of  those  who  have 
written  on  the  subject  of  the  value  of  hive  bees  in  orchards,  appear  to  be  strongly 
in  favour  of  this  practice,  the  tangible  evidence  as  to  their  value  is,  as  has 
already  been  indicated,  rather  meagre  so  far  as  apple  orchards  are  concerned. 
Again  it  is  often  assumed,  on  the  basis  of  little  actual  observation,  that  wild 
bees  are  "scarce."  Many  of  the  species  concerned  are  small  and  inconspicuous, 
and  we  have  repeatedly  checked  the  statement  "  There  isn't  a  bee  in  the  whole 
orchard,"  only  to  find  solitary  bees  present  in  considerable  numbers. 

Obviously,  a  survey  should  be  made  to  determine  the  extent  of  the  wild  bee 
population  before  it  can  be  confidently  stated  that  they  are  "  scarce."  Unfor- 
tunately, investigations  have  never  been  carried  on  for  a  sufficient  length  of  time 
in  one  place  to  determine  the  fluctuations  in  such  populations  that  may  occur 
from  year  to  year;  and  whether,  even  in  favoured  locations,  they  may  not,  occa- 
sionally become  very  scarce.  Generally  speaking,  territory  with  considerable 
waste  land,  rough  pasture,  dykes,  etc.,  will  support  a  considerable  bee  popula- 
tion. The  difference  between  certain  stations  in  the  middle  of  the  Valley  and 
others  near  the  North  Mountain  is  quite  marked.  Even  in  orchards  wholly  or 
partly  in  sod,  there  are  sometimes  places  favourable  to  the  nesting  of  wild  species. 

On  the  other  hand,  certain  soil  types  and  intensive  clean  cultivation,  with  an 
absence  of  rough  land  or  other  favoured  nesting  places  may  possibly  result 
in  an  actual  scarcity  in  all  years,  as  has  been  claimed  by  some  workers.  In 
other  cases,  unfavourable  weather,  natural  enemies  or  other  factors  may  deplete 
the  native  fauna.  Under  such  conditions  there  would  be  no  alternative  to  sup- 
plying the  necessary  number  of  hive  bees  as  a  matter  of  orchard  routine.  Our 
own  observations  in  four  years  out  of  five,  indicate  that  there  was  an  adequate 
population  of  wild  bees  to  produce  a  satisfactory  set  in  most  orchards  studied. 
However,  in  the  fifth  year,  there  were  extremely  unfavourable  conditions  pre- 
vailing during  the  bloom  of  certain  varieties.  Under  these  conditions,  a  much 
heavier  population  of  pollinators  than  would  be  necessary  in  a  normal  year,  would 
be  a  decided  advantage.  Fruit  setting  on  many  varieties  was  very  light,  but 
very  satisfactory  sets  were  obtained  even  with  short  periods  of  exposure,  where 
hive  bees  had  been  properly  distributed  in  adequate  numbers.  The  tendency 
of  the  bees  to  exhibit  greatly  increased  activity,  especially  near  the  colonies, 
during  a  warm,  sunny  period  following  one  of  confinement,  helped  to  bring  this 
about.  The  provision  of  hive  bees,  therefore,  in  any  commercial  orchard  is  a 
measure  of  insurance  against  such  a  condition,  and  should  be  so  regarded.  When 
general  unfavourable  conditions  prevail,  this  must  be  compensated  for  by  a  force 
capable  of  effecting  pollination  in  limited  periods  of  favourable  weather.  The 
question  of  whether  or  not  such  provision  shall  be  made,  resolves  itself  into  a 
question  of  whether  the  cost  of  the  operation  will  be  more  than  compensated  for 
by  the  expected  benefits. 

2.    PROBLEMS    INVOLVED    IN    COLONY    DISTRIBUTION    AND    NUMBER 
(a)  THE    TIME    NECESSARY    FOR    POLLINATION 

The  amount  of  pollination  that  takes  place  varies  with  the  strength  of  the 
colonies  and  with  the  length  of  time  available  for  them  to  do  their  work.  With 
favourable  weather,  a  relatively  small  population  may  accomplish  the  same 
result  as  that  obtained  by  a  much  larger  force  in  a  short  period.  In  considering 
this  question,  the  amount  of  pollination  secured  in  definite  periods  of  time  by  a 
population  of  given  strength  is  of  value. 

At  Kentville  in  1931  we  attempted  a  small  experiment  to  determine  the 
length  of  time  actually  required  for  the  pollination  of  three  Northern  Spy  tree-. 


141 

covering  them  with  tents  and  opening  one  side  of  the  tent  for  1  hour,  5  hours  and 
for  one  day  respectively.  Unfortunately,  the  bee  population  at  this  station 
during  the  period  in  question  was  very  low,  viz.,  only  -028  per  minute — much 
too  low  on  which  to  base  any  calculations.  In  view  of  this  fact  it  is  not  to  be 
expected  that  the  results  would  be  very  consistent.  However,  as  indicated  by 
the  accompanying  table  there  is  a  great  difference  'between  the  tree  that  was 
exposed  during  bloom  and  those  exposed  only  for  a  few  hours.  In  interpreting 
results  from  work  of  this  kind  it  must  be  borne  in  mind  that  the  set  of  fruit 
obtained  is  by  no  means  an  accurate  gauge  of  the  effective  bee  population, 
because  double  visits,  unfruitful  crosses,  and  selfing  must  be  taken  into  account. 
There  is  also  a  very  definite  limit  to  the  number  of  blossoms  that  can  set  fruit  and 
many  blossoms  that  are  pollinated  can  never  produce  mature  fruit.  Therefore, 
figures  showing  the  set  obtained  from  a  given  number  of  blossoms  exposed  to 
pollination  for  a  definite  time  can  have  but  limited  value,  though  it  is  of  interest 
to  study  the  results  obtained,  particularly  from  exposures  of  short  duration, 
since,  in  these  cases,  the  blossoms  pollinated  were  not  in  excess  of  the  number 
that  could  set  under  normal  conditions. 

TABLE  No.  21.— EFFECT  OF  LENGTH  OF  EXPOSURE  ON  POLLINATION 


Tree 


Time  of  exposure 


Number  of 
blossoms 
counted 


Per  cent 
fruit 


1  hour 

5  hours 

All  day 

Throughout  bloom . 


2,553 
2,641 
2,938 
2,552 


0-35 
0  15 
1-29 
5-72 


Note. — Total  acreage  of  orchard  70;  number  of  colonies  37. 

It  will  be  noted  that  the  tree  exposed  throughout  the  day  shows  a  higher  per- 
centage than  the  other  two,  but  the  tree  with  a  five-hour  exposure  shows  a  lower 
percentage  set  than  the  one  exposed  for  only  an  hour.  These  numbers  were  so  low 
that  individual  differences  in  the  tree,  its  location,  number  of  blossoms  and  the 
error  inherent  in  such  a  small  scale  experiment,  might  easily  account  for  the 
differences.  The  experiment  shows  that,  under  these  particular  conditions,  ade- 
quate pollination  was  far  from  being  obtained  from  a  full  day's  exposure. 

During  1932  further  work  was  projected  in  which  it  was  planned  to  expose 
whole  trees  and  individual  limbs  to  pollination  by  bees  for  varying  periods  of 
time,  viz.,  1  hour,  3  hours,  1  day,  2  days,  3  days,  etc.  Unfortunately,  weather 
conditions  interfered  seriously  with  the  experiments.  In  some  cases  the  short 
exposures  were  carried  out  under  ideal  conditions,  but  the  weather  then  became 
wet  and  cold  and  all  activity  ceased  for  days  at  a  time.  The  result  was  that 
an  intended  one-day  exposure  ran  into  several  days,  during  which  no  bees  were 
present.  When  conditions  again  became  suitable  for  flight,  they  were  in  some 
cases  more  and  in  some  less  favourable  than  for  the  shorter  exposures,  with  the 
result  that,  during  the  longer  exposures,  there  were  in  some  cases  actually  a 
lower  number  of  bees  available  than  during  the  shorter  periods.  Interpretation 
of  results  therefore  becomes  very  difficult.  Bearing  in  mind  these  points,  the 
results  as  indicated  in  the  accompanying  table,  show  no  more  irregularity  than 
would  be  expected  from  the  nature  of  such  tests.  In  this  tabulation  we  have 
calculated  the  number  of  "  effective  hours "  that  the  blossoms  were  exposed. 
This  was  arrived  at  by  taking  the  total  number  of  ten-minute  periods  during 
the  time  of  exposure  of  that  particular  bloom,  when  any  number  of  bees,  how- 
ever small,  was  found  at  work  in  that  particular  orchard.  This  figure,  in  turn, 
was  divided  by  six.  Since,  however,  the  number  present  during  the  period  of 
exposure  was  an  equally  important  factor,  we  have  also  calculated  the  average 
number  of  bees  taken  per  ten-minute  count  over  the  same  period. 


142 


Allowing  for  the  irregularities  inherent  in  such  tests,  and  without  attempt- 
ing to  carry  conclusions  from  such  figures  too  far,  we  find  that  the  set  obtained 
in  as  short  a  period  as  one  hour  was  sufficient  to  give  a  commercial  crop  in  the 
case  of  Gravenstein,  and  that  the  set  obtained  in  five  hours  and  upwards  of 
effective  exposure,  with  suitable  conditions  for  good  activity  of  the  bees,  in 
Gravenstein  and  King  was  great  enough  to  require  thinning.  Had  the  entire 
tree  been  exposed  in  Mcintosh,  greater  sets  undoubtedly  would  have  been 
obtained  on  the  tented  trees,  since  these  showed  a  set  on  the  exposed  side  notice- 
ably in  excess  of  that  obtained  on  the  unexposed  side.  The  comparatively  low 
sets  of  fruit  on  all  exposures  in  the  case  of  Blenheim  were  obtained  in  an  orchard 
in  which  cross-unfruitful  and  self-unfruitful  varieties  were  mixed,  viz.,  Stark, 
Mann  and  Blenheim,  and  was  one  in  which  poor  crops  were  the  general  rule. 
Wagener  and  other  bouquets  were  supplied,  but  this,  undoubtedly,  did  not  prevent 
a  great  deal  of  unfruitful  cross-  and  self-pollination. 

Allowing  for  the  proportion  of  nectar  carriers,  duplicate  visits,  unsuccessful 
crosses,  etc.,  these  figures  do  not  appear  inconsistent  with  the  information 
obtained  from  other  sources. 


TABLE  No.  22. 


-TABLE  SHOWING  INFLUENCE  OF  LENGTH  OF  EXPOSURE  ON 
POLLINATION 


Variety 

Number    of 

blossoms 

used 

Number  of 
effective 

hours  (*) 
of  exposure 

Average 
activity 

per  10 
minutes 

during 
period  of 
effective 
exposure 

Per  cent 

set 
obtained 

Per  cent 
fruit  after 
July  drop 

1,599 
2,267 
2,312 
2,382 
2,547 
2,658 
2,141 

909 
1,556 
1,631 
1,363 
2,086 
1,247 
1,393 

3,325 
2,496 
2,881 
2,311 

1,174 
1,007 
1,268 
1,401 
1,870 

nil 
1 
5 

HI 
23§ 
Duration   of 
bloom, 
nil 

4| 

Hi 

22| 

23$ 

Duration  of 

bloom. 

5 

I 

21 
4| 
Duration  of 
bloom . 

nil 

1 

4 

5* 

Duration  of 
bloom. 

11-69(2) 

9-97 
18-47 
11-92 

20-77 
18-59 
26-86 

1-36 

5-98 
27-16 
25-61 
39-26 
28-63 
19  02 

4-69 

6-86 

6-98 

13-85 

0  085 
119 
0-47 
0-21 

7-54 

1-63 

16-78 
15-97 
12-35 
801 
5-31 

3-97 

et 

7  01 

« 

6-76 

u 

10-91 

« 

9-63 

u 

10-28 

0-25 

204 
103 
2-79 
5-38 
5  05 

1-99 

« 

6-72 

« 

10-93 

" 

7-19 

u 

9-70 

a 

11-84 

Mcintosh  (0 

200 
3-44 
3-29 

3  91 

5-61 

it 

5-73 

a 

8-57 

0  085 

131 
2- 12 
1-98 

010 

» 

008 

a 

0-21 

u 

ti-L'li 

(*)  An  effective  hour  represents  6  ten-minute  counts,  during  which  the  blossoms  were  exposed  and 
during  which  some  bee  activity  was  actually  observed  in  the  orchard. 
0)  Intent. 
(2)  High  original  set  probably  produced  by  selfing  from  rubbing  about  inside  the  bag. 

(b)  TYPE  OF  COLONY  REQUIRED 

(i)  Historical.  Most  writers  favour  the  use  of  strong  overwintered  colonies 
for  pollination  purposes.  Some,  however,  recommend  the  use  of  package  bees 
when  others  are  not  available,  while  a  few  go  so  far  as  to  claim  that  certain 
types  of  packages  are  preferable  and  have  a  larger  field  force  than  overwintered 
colonies.     The  following  recommendations  may  be  regarded  as  typical: 


143 


DeOng  (1925)  recommends  that  bees  which  are  to  be  used  for  pollination 
should  be  in  good  condition  and  strong  in  numbers  in  the  spring,  though,  for 
purely  honey  producing  hives,  large  numbers  in  early  spring  are  not  so  necessary. 


ll 
It 

<0^ 

lit 

w 

<0  v*s 

si 

I 
I 

1 

1 
I 

1 

! 

bio 

O 

X> 

1 

ll 

s 

^ 

faO 

] 

S 

,  ^ 

S 

i 

1—1 

1 

^ 

cr> 

■ 
1 

1 

1 

1 

1 

- 

^ 

■ 

o 
^2 

i 

■ 

§ 

lO 

o 

t^ 

I 

■ 
1 

1 

■ 

i 

■ 

2 

3 

lO 

IO 
CO 

§ 

>o 

£ 

on 

to 

(V 

1 

1 

1 

■ 

■ 

j6 
o 

r~  ■ 

(V 

R 

>o 

s 

■  i 

1 

CO 

<=> 

CO 

* 

^ 

-Si 

O 

°i 

Od 

€=>    H 

1 

€=3 

<fr 

Si 

CO 

C\J 

oo  - 

1 

I 
i 

2 

X 

I  * 

3 

fi: 

C=9 
IO   ■ 

1 

s 

'o 

(V 

d 

rO 

i 

o 

o 

(V 

°  -  3: 

5 

£ 

■p 

3j 

8 

iJD 

O 

*2  =  >0 

>o 

i 

<o 

+3 

5 

o 

Q 

5 

<=> 

^ 

3 

3 

>o 

"o 

" 

= 

§ 

DO 

3 

i 

=? 

o 

CD 

o> 

02 

o 

- 

+J 

- 

H3 

o 

^ 

o 

■ 

N 

faC 

<al 

k 

i? 

C5 

o 

* 

^ 

1 

? 

■4J 

1 

: 

rt 

Q 

3 

■ 

>o 

■ 

1. 

00 

CO 

5 

d 

<* 

! 

*>                   V                  S                  Xs                   K                   "o 

^ 

5 

•< 

i 

Philp  and  Vansell  (1932)  in  connection  with  the  use  of  bees  for  pollination, 
point  out  that  only  strong  colonies  will  prove  of  much  value  during  the  early 
spring,  when  a  large  number  of  bees  are  required  in  the  hive  to  maintain  the 
brood  nest  temperature  at  95°  F. 


144 

Phillips  (1930)  advises  that  strong  colonies  only  should  be  used  in  the 
orchard,  since  a  hive  with  twelve  frames  of  brood  is  four  times  as  valuable  as 
one  with  six  frames  of  brood.  For  this  reason  he  considers  that  the  colony 
should  be  in  two  hive  bodies.  The  strength  of  the  colonies  is  regarded  as  more 
important  than  their  number  by  this  worker,  since  powerful  colonies  send  out 
proportionately  far  more  bees  than  weak  ones,  and  also  start  flight  sooner. 
While  no  hard  and  fast  rule  is  laid  down,  the  author  suggests  one  twelve-frame 
colony  to  each  four  acres,  or  one  six-frame  colony  per  acre. 

Murneek  (1930)  considers  that  a  good  colony  should  contain  at  least  five 
pounds  of  bees,  or  approximately  25,000. 

A  comparison  was  made  by  Hutson  (1928)  between  the  numerical  strengths 
of  package  bees  and  overwintered  colonies  by  weighing  the  entire  colony,  remov- 
ing the  bees,  then  weighing  the  empty  hive — ten  being  used  in  each  case.  On  the 
basis  of  5,000  bees  to  the  pound,  the  bees  in  ten  hives  were  said  to  comprise 
108,750  individuals  as  compared  with  150,000  for  ten  3-pound  packages — an 
advantage  of  40,000  over  the  overwintered  colonies.  It  was  also  shown  that  over- 
wintered colonies  were  very  unequal  in  strength.  On  the  basis  of  the  foregoing,  it 
was  concluded  that  3-pound  package  bees  were  superior  to  overwintered  colonies 
on  the  score  of  strength  and  uniformity,  thus  giving  the  orchardist  an  opportunity 
to  secure  better  distribution  in  his  orchard. 

Other  New  Jersey  experiments  (1929  and  1930)  showed  that  combless 
packages,  regardless  of  size,  were  not  comparable  in  value  to  overwintered  colon- 
ies; and  that  the  orchard  packages,  while  superior  to  combless  packages,  were 
also  inferior  to  overwintered  colonies. 

Filmer  (1931  and  1932)  has  presented  some  evidence  regarding  the  value 
of  different  types  of  colonies.  The  packages  used  were  established  three  weeks 
before  the  counts  were  made,  which  were  carried  out,  presumably,  during  fruit 
bloom.  Packages  established  on  foundation  were  fed  40  pounds  of  "one  to  one" 
sugar  syrup.  From  a  study  of  the  data  presented  in  this  paper  the  following 
may  be  gathered: — 

,1.  Total  bees  show  no  correlation  with  total  pollen  gatherers  which  vary 
greatly  in  different  types  of  colonies. 

2.  Total  bees  for  overwintered  colonies  and  for  package^  supplied  with 
foundation  only,  is  the  same;  for  3-pound  packages  on  drawn  comb  it  is  less. 

3.  Considering  the  different  weights  of  colonies  in  relation  to  total  activity. 
2-  and  3-pound  packages  seem  little,  if  any,  inferior  to  6-pound  packages  of 
the  same  type.  In  fact  the  6-pound  package  on  drawn  comb  shows  less  activity 
than  the  corresponding  2-pound  package.  The  wrapped  3-pound  package  showed 
only  50  per  cent  of  the  activity  of  packages  supplied  with  drawn  comb,  and  43 
per  cent  of  3-pound  overwintered  colonies. 

4.  The  average  pollen  gatherers  liberated  from  2-,  3-  and  6-pound  packages, 
whether  those  supplied  with  drawn  comb  or  with  foundation  alone,  showed  little 
significant  difference,  but  were  decidedly  less  efficient  than  overwintered  colonies; 
while  wrapped  packages  showed  negligible  efficiency. 

5.  Total  pollen  carriers  are  greatest,  in  all  cases,  from  the  overwintered 
colonies;  for  3-pound  packages  the  count  strongly  favour-  the  packages  supplied 
with  drawn  comb  in  one  case,  with  result  about  the  same  in  the  other. 

6.  On  the  basis  of  the  data  secured,  the  author  considered  that  there  was  a 
relation  between  the  brood  content  and  total  activity  in  colonies  of  the  same 
weight,  with  bees  on  foundation  showing  a  greater  activity  in  relation  to  brood 
area  than  those  on  drawn  combs.  He  considered  that  the  figure  for  total  bees 
was  of  more  value  than  that  of  pollen  carriers  alone,  owing  to  the  great  van- 
ability  in  numbers  of  pollen  carriers. 


145 


Farrar  (1931)  has  brought  forward  evidence  bearing  on  the  question  of  the 
relative  value  of  overwintered  colonies  as  compared  with  package  bees.  He  notes 
that  considerable  emphasis  has  been  placed  upon  the  necessity  of  designing  a 
"  colony  unit "  that  the  fruit  grower  can  use  without  a  knowledge  of  bees  and 
with  a  minimum  of  handling.  He  points  out  that,  upon  this  development, 
depends  the  question  of  whether  to  stimulate  practical  beekeeping  or  to  depend 
upon  packages.  He  considers  that  the  orchard  yield  is  the  result,  of  too  great  a 
complex  of  factors  to  be  a  suitable  criterion  of  the  colony's  efficiency  in  pol- 
lination, but  the  degree  of  pollination  should  be  proportional  to  the  number  of 
bees  visiting  the  blossoms.  He  assumes  that  the  colony  unit  furnishing  the 
greatest  number  of  field  bees  per  minute  will  provide  visits  to  the  greatest 
number  of  blossoms,  and  therefore,  accomplish  the  greatest  results  in  pollination. 
He  describes  the  results  of  two  years  experiments  at  Amherst,  Mass.,  on  the 
number  of  bees  furnished  during  bloom  by  (1)  package  bees,  (2)  nuclei,  and  (3) 
overwintering  colonies,  which  indicate  a  pronounced  advantage  in  favour  of  the 
last.  In  a  normal  season  (1929)  the  overwintered  colonies  furnished  8  to  20  times 
as  many  bees  per  minute  as  3-pound  packages  or  3-frame  nuclei,  when  the  bees 
were  allowed  to  fly  from  their  shipping  package.  In  1930,  under  abnormally 
favourable  conditions,  normal  overwintered  colonies  showed  a  decided  advantage 
over  5-pound  packages,  and  these  in  turn  over  smaller  packages.  The  writer 
concludes  that  where  colonies  cannot  be  secured,  strong  packages  should  be 
obtained  a  week  in  advance  of  expected  blooming  date,  installed  in  hives — prefer- 
ably on  drawn  comb — and  fed,  in  order  to  insure  immediate  establishment  of  the 
brood  nest. 

Woodrow  (1932)  has  presented  some  valuable  data  respecting  the  relative 
value  of  different  types  of  colonies  using  the  method  of  Farrar  (loc.  cit.),  the 
counts  being  made  during  apple  bloom.  Records  were  based  on  the  number  of 
bees  leaving  the  hive,  made  up  of  3  pounds  of  adult  bees,  one  3-pound  package 
on  comb  foundation,  and  one  3-pound  package  in  its  shipping  cage.  The  second 
was  similar  to  the  first,  but  consisted  of  5-pound  units.  The  third  group 
consisted  of  unmodified  overwintered  colonies,  one  with  3  frames  of  brood  and 
1-63  pounds  of  bees;  one  with  7  frames  of  brood  and  4-38  pounds  of  bees;  and  one 
of  16  frames  of  brood  and  8-25  pounds  of  bees. 

Flight  from  5-pound  units  exceeded  those  from  3-pound  units,  though  not  in 
proportion  to  the  strength  of  the  colonies.  A  study  of  the  flight  figures  showed 
that,  in  general,  at  any  given  temperature,  the  flight  of  any  colony  wTas  roughly 
proportional  to  its  strength ;  but  the  weaker  colonies  flew  less  freely  at  the  lower 
temperatures  and  more  freely  at  the  higher  temperatures.  It  is  concluded  from 
the  results  of  these  studies,  that  strong  overwintered  colonies  are  superior  to 
package  bees,  but  that  if  only  weak  colonies  are  available,  3-  or  5-pound 
packages  are  more  desirable,  since  these  can  be  obtained  for  the  same  price.  In 
this  work  the  total  number  of  bees  issuing  from  the  hive  was  used  with  no  dis- 
tinction between  pollen  and  nectar  gatherers. 

(ii)  Observations  on  Strength  of  Different  Types  of  Colonies. — At  Ottawa  in 
1931,  four  overwintered  colonies  of  average  strength  were  measured  for  brood, 
potential  force  in  bees,  and  total  force.  The  results  are  indicated  in  the  following 
table: 

TABLE  No.  23.— STRENGTH  OF  OVERWINTERED  COLONIES  MAY  6,  1931 


Colony  number 

Brood  in 
square 
inches 

Potential 
force  of 
bees  (*) 

Weight 
of  bees 

Total 

force 

in  bees 

313 

650-52 
805-38 
890-03 
725-90 

16,260 
20,134 
22,250 
18  147 

lbs.      oz. 

3  11 
5        11 
5        12 

4  4 

18,437 

316 

28,437 

327 

28,750 

320 

21,250 

(*)  The  potential  force  refers  to  the  number  of  bees  that  would  emerge  from  their  cells  within  the 
next  21  days. 
60796—10 


146 

The  above  table  shows  that  all  overwintered  colonies  contained  a  stronger- 
force  of  bees  than  did  the  2-pound  or  3-pound  packages  which  arrived 
May  5,  though  definite  figures  for  the  latter  are  not  available.  Furthermore,  all 
were  stronger  than  3-pound  packages,  and  two  of  them  stronger  than  5- 
pound  packages,  and  in  addition  all  contained  brood,  which  the  packages 
received  did  not.  The  total  force  and  the  field  force  of  the  same  colonies  were 
taken  during  apple  bloom  as  indicated  in  table  No.  24. 


TABLE  No.  24. 


-STRENGTH  OF  OVERWINTERED  COLONIES  DURING  FRUIT  BLOOM, 

1931 


Colony  number 

Brood  in 
square 
inches 

Potential 
force  of 
of  bees 

Total 

force 

in  bees 

Field 
force 

313 

1246 • 18 
1279-56 
1262-88 
1130-76 

31,154 
31,989 
31,572 

28,269 

25,937 
31,875 
28,750 
21,875 

16,875 

316 

14,062 

327 

18,750 

320 

10,937 

The  above  were  actually  measured  by  weight  and  the  figures  based  on  5,000 
to  a  pound. 

It  will  be  noted  that  the  field  force  of  each  of  the  overwintered  colonies 
alone,  exceeded  the  total  force  of  bees  in  a  2-pound  package  while  two  of  them 
exceeded  a  3-pound  package. 

The  colony  strength,  and  more  particularly  the  strength  of  the  field  force, 
is  a  most  important  consideration  in  the  use  of  hive  bees  for  pollination  pur- 
poses. Our  own  information  indicated  that  the  working  force  is  much  greater, 
both  actually  and  proportionally,  in  a  strong  colony  than  in  a  weak  one.  It 
appears  that  not  only  do  more  bees  leave  the  hive,  but  they  make  longer  flights 
and  will  work  when  weather  conditions  are  less  favourable.  These  facts  were 
very  apparent  throughout  the  entire  investigation.  Furthermore,  anything  that 
tended  to  weaken  the  colony,  such  as  persistent  unfavourable  weather  and  lack 
of  stores  resulting  in  a  break  in  brood  rearing,  or  cases  of  poisoning  from  sprays 
or  dusts,  quickly  reduced  the  activity  noted  in  the  orchard.  Beekeepers  ordi- 
narily do  not  depend  upon  apple  bloom  for  a  surplus  of  honey,  but  are  content 
to  have  their  colonies  at  the  peak  in  time  for  the  clover  flow.  For  apple  pollina- 
tion purposes  this  is  not  sufficient  and  special  means  must  be  taken  to  get  the 
colonies  into  optimum  condition  by  the  time  the  apples  blossom. 

(iii)  Experimental. — In  1932,  it  was  decided  to  make  a  comparison  of  the 
performance,  during  pollination,  of  the  different  types  of  colonies.  Unfortunately 
the  overwintered  colonies  were  extremely  weak  and,  owing  to  unfavourable 
weather,  the  packages,  instead  of  increasino-  in  strength,  actually  dwindled  in 
numbers  of  individuals.  In  the  test  there  were  four  overwintered  colonies  and 
two  each  of  2-pound,  3-pound  and  5-pound  packages.  The  latter  were  received 
on  April  26  in  excellent  condition. 

They  were  released  on  drawn  combs  as  soon  as  they  arrived,  the  weather 
being  very  cool,  and  some  rain  mixed  with  snow  was  falling.  The  queen  accom- 
panying one  of  the  3-pound  packages  was  found  dead,  but  was  immediately 
replaced  with  a  queen  from  another  package.  The  overwintered  colonies  that 
were  to  be  used  as  checks  against  the  packages  were  still  packed  in  their  winter 
cases.  Therefore,  the  package  bees  were  released  into  protected  hives  to  make 
conditions  comparable. 

On  April  29,  the  force  of  bees  in  each  of  the  overwintered  colonic-  was 
weighed  and  calculated  on  the  basis  of  5,000  bees  to  the  pound.  The  results  were 
as  follows: — 


147 


Colony  number 

Weight  of 
bees 

Total 
force 

Colony  number 

Weight  of 
bees 

Total 
force 

241 

287 

lbs.     oz. 

2        14 
1          7 

14,375 
7,187 

240 

257 

lbs.      oz. 

2          7 
2           4| 

12,187 
11,406 

Although  the  colonies  selected  were  of  good  average  strength  for  the  season, 
the  above  figures  show  that  they  were  very  little  stronger  in  bees  than  normal 
2-pound  packages,  and  weaker  than  the  5-pound  packages.  The  colonies,  how- 
ever, already  had  brood  in  their  hives,  some  of  which  would  emerge  before  the 
apple  bloom  opened,  while  the  packages  had  none. 

On  April  30,  this  brood  was  measured  and  found  to  be  as  follows: — 

Colonv  numbers 241  287  240  257 

Square  inches  of  brood 417-02  270-37  441-77  408-37 

Potential  force  in  bees  (*) 10.425  6,759  11,066  10,209 

(*)  The  potential  force  in  bees  represents  the  number  that  would  emerge  within  the  next  21  days. 

On  May  19,  two  apple  trees  in  the  bee  yard  were  in  bloom  but  none  had 
opened  in  the  main  orchards.  Another  measurement  in  bees  and  brood  was 
made  on  this  date  and  this  is  shown  in  table  25.  The  field  force  of  the  colonies 
was  not  determined  at  this  time. 

The  figures  in  table  25  show  that  all  four  overwintered  colonies  had 
gained  in  strength  both  in  bees  and  brood  since  the  counts  of  April  29  and  30; 
but  the  gain  in  bees  in  colonies  240  and  257  was  very  slight.  It  will  also  be  seen 
that  all  packages  were  reduced  in  strength  of  bees,  the  greatest  reduction  in 
each  group  equalled  46-88  per  cent  for  the  2-pound  packages,  47-92  for  the 
3-pound  packages  and  36-25  for  the  5-pound  packages.  Even  with  this  loss 
the  5-pound  packages  still  had  a  larger  force  of  bees  than  the  overwintered 
colonies,  at  the  commencement  of  apple  bloom.  It  should  be  pointed  out  here, 
however,  that  because  of  poor  wintering  and  heavy  dwindling  during  the  early 
spring,  the  overwintered  colonies  were  much  weaker  in  1932  than  they  had 
been  for  the  past  16  years.  On  the  same  date  in  1931  four  average  colonies 
contained  an  average  of  5  pounds  6|  ounces  or  approximately  27,109  bees.  It 
will  be  seen,  therefore,  that  the  comparison  was  made  under  abnormal  condi- 
tions, affecting  the  overwintered  colonies  particularly,  though  as  noted  already 
the  packages  were  considerably  weaker  than  their  designated  strength. 

On  May  20,  the  day  after  the  measurements  shown  in  table  25  were 
taken,  it  was  estimated  that  about  5  per  cent  bloom  was  open  in  the  main 
orchards. 


TABLE  No.  25.— TABLE  SHOWING  THE  APPROXIMATE  NUMBER  OF  BEES  AND 
THE  AMOUNT  OF  BROOD  IN  OVERWINTERED  COLONIES  AND  PACKAGES 
ON  MAY  18-19,  1932. 


Colony  number 

Overwintered  colonies 

2-pound 
packages 

3-pound 
packages 

5-pound 
packages 

241 

287 

240 

257 

305 

319 

211 

309 

333 

334 

1,090-50 
27,262 
16,250 

610-64 
15,266 
9,062 

878-47 
21,962 
12,812 

829-74 
20.743 
11,562 

541-14 
13,528 
6,250 

466-50 
11,662 
5,312 

738-85 
18,471 
7,812 

727-67 
18,192 
11,562 

768-67 
19.217 
19,062 

736-66 

18,416 

Total  force  of  bees  in  colonies 

15,937 

The  apple  bloom  lasted  for  about  12  days,  as  it  was  practically  all  gone  on 
May  31.  No  further  measurements  of  bees  or  brood  were  made,  but  counts  of 
bees  returning  to  the  hives  were  made  for  2  and  4  minute  periods  whenever 
weather  conditions  permitted  during  the  time  of  bloom,  according  to  the  trapping 
method  devised  by  Farrar    (1931).     During  the  period  under  review  records 

60796—10* 


148 

of  the  activity  of  over  7,000  bees  were  made.  The  results  were  averaged  to  show 
the  comparative  number  per  minute  of  total  bees  and  pollen  gatherers  liberated 
by  each  type  of  colony.    These  data  are  presented  in  table  26. 

TABLE  No.  26.— NUMBER  OF  BEES  LIBERATED  PER  MINUTE  FROM  DIFFERENT 

TYPES  OF  COLONIES 


Type  of  colony 


Average 

pollen 
gatherers 
liberated 
per  minute 


Average 

total 

liberated 

per 

minute 


Average 

total  force 

in  bees 


Average 

potential 

force  in 

bees 


Overwintered . . . 
2-pound  package 
3-pound  package 
5-pound  package 


8-46 
2-11 
8-51 
5-56 


35-89 

6-52 

31-56 

38-20 


12,421-5 

5,781 

9,687 
17,499-5 


21,308-25 
12,595 
18,663 
18,816-5 


This  table  indicates  little  significant  difference  between  the  different  types  of 
colonies,  except  that  the  2-pound  packages  are  much  inferior  on  all  counts.  The 
fact  that  the  5-pound  packages,  though  stronger  in  field  force  and  in  total  numbers 
liberated,  are  lower  in  pollen  gatherers,  may  or  may  not  be  significant.  That  the 
potential  force  of  the  5-pound  packages  is  little  different  from  that  of  3-pound 
packages,  in  spite  of  their  greater  force  of  bees  at  this  time,  is  worthy  of  note. 
The  obvious  weakness  of  the  overwintered  colonies,  averaging  only  about  2^ 
pounds,  makes  the  data  worthless  as  an  indication  of  the  performance  of  strong 
colonies.  Nevertheless,  the  figures  are  presented  for  what  they  may  be  worth. 
Since,  for  both  2-pound  and  3-pound  packages  the  total  force  was  less  than  the 
field  force  for  a  good  overwintered  colony,  the  superior  value  of  the  latter,  for 
pollination  purposes,  would  appear  to  be  obvious. 

(c)  FLIGHT  AND  CONCENTRATION  OF  BEES 

(i)  General. — A  great  deal  has  been  written  regarding  the  distance  of  bee 
flight  and  the  problems  involved  in  the  point  at  which  maximum  concentration 
occurs.  It  is  usually  stated  that  bees  will  fly  a  mile  or  two  from  the  hive  for 
nectar  or  pollen  and  even  much  greater  distances  have  been  recorded.  The 
maximum  distance  bees  will  fly  is  determined  by  many  factors,  <ome  of  which 
are  considered  in  the  following  pages. 

Certain  workers  appear  to  believe  that  there  is  a  point  located  at  a  definite 
distance  from  the  hives,  at  which  the  greatest  concentration  of  bees  will  take 
place.  Hutson  (1926)  considers  that  this  point  is  15  feet  from  the  hives,  gradually 
diminishing  as  the  distance  increases,  and  that  this,  at  least  in  certain  cases,  car. 
be  definitely  correlated  with  the  set  of  fruit  obtained.  On  the  other  hand,  many 
workers  contend  that  bees  tend  to  concentrate  at  a  much  greater  distance  than 
stated  by  Hutson.  For  example,  MacDaniels  (1931)  describes  experiments  in 
which  it  is  shown  that  bees  working  out  of  the  hive  do  not  stop  close  to  the  hive, 
but  rather  go  directly  to  some  distant  spot.  It  was  further  concluded  by  this 
worker  that  the  placing  of  colonies  of  bees  with  relation  to  the  varieties  to  be 
pollinated  is  of  less  importance  than  the  position  of  the  pollen  source  in  relation 
to  the  trees  to  be  pollinated.  Our  counts  and  observation-  gave  no  encouragement 
to  the  belief  that  there  is  a  definite  mathematical  point  at  any  certain  distance 
from  the  colonies  at  which  maximum  concentration  will  occur.  There  is.  how- 
ever, considerable  evidence  supporting  the  contention  that  the  optimum  working 
distance  is  not  necessarily  close  to  the  hives.  In  this  connection,  observations 
made  at  Coaldale,  Alberta,  are  of  interest.  On  a  fine  bright  day.  in  an  apiary 
located  on  the  edge  of  an  alfalfa  field,  with  nectar  secretion  abundant  and  meat 
activity  at  the  hives,  the  bees  were  going  some  distance  for  their  supplies  and  a 
search  of  the  particular  field  in  which  the  hives  were  located  showed  only  an 
occasional  bee  in  the  blossoms,  though  more  distant  fields  were  freely  visite 


149 

It  would  appear  that,  while  the  bees  are,  in  general,  found  in  greatest 
numbers  near  the  hives,  the  distance  of  flight  and  the  point  of  maximum  con- 
centration are  variable  and  are  conditioned  by  a  number  of  factors  of  which  (i) 
the  condition  of  bloom,  (ii)  varying  varietal  attractiveness,  (iii)  the  amount  and 
mass  of  bloom  available  in  relation  to  the  bee  population,  (iv)  the  availability  of 
other  kinds  of  bloom,  (v)  the  physical  features  of  the  area  concerned,  (vi)  the 
prevailing  weather  conditions,  (vii)  the  position  of  the  colonies,  and  (viii)  the 
habit  of  bees  of  working  in  a  limited  area,  are  a  few  of  those  that  must  be  taken 
into  account.    These  are  discussed  in  the  following  sections. 

(ii)  Condition  of  Bloom. — The  condition  of  the  bloom,  particularly  with 
regard  to  nectar  secretion  and  the  availability  of  free  pollen,  evidently  has  an 
influence  on  the  activity  of  bees  on  that  particular  variety.  This  was  very 
noticeable  throughout  the  course  of  our  observations.  What  appears  to  be  a  good 
example  of  this  fact  is  recorded  in  our  notes  for  1930.  The  observation  was  made 
on  Long  island  where  an  apiary  of  25  strong  colonies  was  placed  just  north  of  an 
orchard  in  full  bloom.  Other  orchards,  to  an  extent  of  about  ninety  acres, 
extended  westward  for  a  distance  of  two  miles,  there  being  no  orchards  on  the 
east.  Four  men  stationed  in  the  orchard  adjoining  the  colonies  took  observations 
throughout  the  day,  but  recorded  only  a  negligible  number  of  bees.  During  the 
afternoon  two  men  scouted  the  entire  island  examining,  besides  apple,  the  blos- 
soms of  oak,  rhododendron,  blueberry,  horse  chestnut,  dandelion,  clover,  Siberian 
pea,  star  of  Bethlehem,  plum,  Labrador  tea,  lilac,  mountain  ash,  etc.,  without 
finding  any  bees.  Returning  to  the  apiary,  where  the  bees  were  still  working 
actively,  a  flight  of  bees  could  be  seen  flying  westward.  This  line  of  flight 
led  to  a  back  orchard  of  Ben  Davis  trees  in  full  bloom,  on  an  adjoining  farm 
about  \  mile  distant  from  the  colonies. 

Upon  approaching  this  orchard  a  strong  aroma  of  apple  blossoms  and  a 
distinct  hum  of  bees  could  be  detected,  at  a  considerable  distance  from 
the  trees.  The  first  two  counts,  begun  at  4.20  and  4.30,  gave  counts  of  39  and 
23  respectively,  and  the  blossoms  were  obviously  swarming  with  bees.  This 
appeared  the  more  remarkable  as  the  sky  had  become  overcast  with  heavy 
clouds,  though  the  temperature  registered  about  76°  F.  In  the  intervening 
rough  pasture  between  the  apiary  and  this  orchard,  bees  were  observed  on 
rhododendron  and  blueberry. 

The  next  day  bees  were  still  abundant  in  this  orchard,  but,  unlike  the  day 
before,  were  much  more  evenly  distributed,  and  also  in  the  orchard  contiguous 
to  the  apiary.  They  were  even  found  in  measurable  numbers  in  an  orchard 
|  mile  west,  and  small  numbers  were  taken  on  buttercup,  dandelion,  oak,  and 
chokeberry,  the  weather,  like  that  of  the  previous  day,  being  generally  quite 
favourable  for  flight  and  activity. 

(iii)  Varietal  Attractiveness. — A  serious  attempt  was  made  to  determine 
whether  or  not  different  varieties  of  bloom  vary  in  their  attractiveness  to  bees. 
Certain  varieties  that  show  few  bee  visitors  on  certain  days,  at  others  will  be 
found  to  have  many  such  visitors  for  reasons  indicated  in  the  preceding  section. 
It  is,  therefore,  dangerous  to  draw  too  sweeping  conclusions  without  a  great  many 
careful  observations  extending  over  the  entire  blossoming  period.  We  are  satisfied 
that  some  varieties  are  more  attractive  than  others;  but  definite  proof  is  difficult, 
owing  to  the  fact  that  varieties  cannot  be  compared  on  an  equal  basis — no  two 
being  in  exactly  the  same  condition  of  attractiveness  with  respect  to  nectar 
secretion,  etc.,  when  counts  are  taken — and  varying  weather  conditions  make 
observations  under  identical  conditions  impossible.  At  the  same  time,  certain 
varieties  are  outstanding  in  this  respect,  notably  Gravenstein,  which  always 
gave  large  counts  when  in  proper  condition.  The  fact  that  few  other  varieties 
are  open  when  Gravenstein  is  in  bloom  would  tend  toward  a  greater  concentration 
on  this  variety.     Gravenstein  has  also  a  showy  blossom,  comparatively  good 


150 

nectar  secretion  and  more  pollen  than  most  triploid  varieties.  Golden  Russet 
is  another  variety  that  seems  to  be  favoured  by  both  wild  and  hive  bees  and, 
with  both  these  varieties,  blossom  visitors  seem  to  persist  throughout  the  bloom 
to  a  greater  extent  than  with  some  others.  Though  Golden  Russet  has  a  much 
less  showy  blossom  than  Gravenstein  it  produces  an  abundance  of  pollen  and, 
hence,  is  popular  with  pollen  gatherers.  Without  much  more  careful  work,  how- 
ever, it  is  not  possible  to  be  more  definite  regarding  attractiveness  of  varieties. 
It  is  worthy  of  note  that  certain  varieties,  e.g.,  Blenheim  and  Stark,  seem  to  have 
greater  difficulty  in  getting  cross-pollinated  than  other  equally  self-unfruitful 
varieties,  e.g.,  Spy  and  Cox  Orange,  under  very  similar  conditions.  The  former 
two  varieties  furnish  comparatively  small  quantities  of  pollen,  while  the  latter 
are  notable  for  the  comparatively  large  amounts  that  they  produce;  this  may 
affect  the  number  of  insect  visitors. 

(iv)  Effect  of  Masses  of  Bloom. — The  foregoing  sections  emphasize  the 
important  effect  of  the  condition  and  nature  of  bloom  in  attracting  bees.  The 
attractiveness,  at  certain  periods  of  bloom  of  an  orchard  of  Ben  Davis  trees  was 
particularly  mentioned.  Other  Ben  Davis  trees,  as  well  as  those  of  other 
varieties  in  a  similar  condition  of  bloom,  were  present  in  the  orchard  contiguous 
to  the  apiary  in  question,  though  not  massed  to  the  same  extent.  The  effect  of 
masses  of  bloom  in  a  correct  condition  to  attract  bees  has  been  noted  on  other 
occasions.  One  orchard  in  which  bees  were  placed  for  two  different  years  was 
situated  at  the  base  of  the  North  Mountain  and  consisted  of  moderate  sized 
trees,  mostly  Baldwin,  Stark  and  Blenheim.  This  orchard  had  been  heavily 
pruned  and  there  was  not  present  any  great  mass  of  bloom  at  any  one  time. 
Seven  colonies  were  present  in  the  orchard  which  was  of  seven  acres  in  extent 
and,  though  we  now  know  that  this  was  insufficient  for  an  area  otherwise  totally 
devoid  of  bees,  the  fact  that  on  many  visits  wre  could  rarely  find  any  bees  what- 
ever in  the  bloom,  was  somewhat  puzzling.  During  one  of  these  visits  in  which 
no  bees  could  be  detected,  though  considerable  activity  was  observable  at  the 
hives,  an  orchard  J  of  a  mile  away  was  found  to  contain  considerable  numbers. 
The  second  orchard  was  below  the  level  of  the  first,  was  unpruned  and  generally 
uncared  for,  but  presented  dense  masses  of  bloom  which  gave  off  an  aroma 
distinctly  noticeable  to  one  passing  along  the  adjoining  road.  Varietal  attrac- 
tiveness may  also  have  been  a  factor  in  this  particular  case.  The  greater 
attractiveness  of  larger  masses  is  also  evident  in  the  difficulty,  sometimes  experi- 
enced, of  getting  bees  to  visit  orchard  bouquets,  since  they  definitely  prefer  to 
go  to  the  greater  masses  of  bloom,  other  things  being  equal. 

(v)  Availability  of  Other  Bloom. — Fortunately,  since  apples  do  not  appear 
to  be  as  attractive  to  bees  as  many  other  species  of  plants,  there  are  not  usually 
available  at  blossom  time,  other  large  sources  of  pollen  or  nectar.  However, 
there  are  a  number  of  species  of  plants  present  in  smaller  numbers.  On  many 
occasions,  when  it  was  difficult  to  find  bees  in  the  apple  bloom,  their  presence 
in  large  numbers  on  strawberries,  Siberian  pea,  dandelions,  etc.,  could  be  readily 
detected.  On  one  occasion,  when  careful  search  failed  to  reveal  bees  in  appreci- 
able numbers  more  than  a  quarter  of  a  mile  from  the  apiary  on  apple  bloom, 
they  were  counted  in  large  numbers  on  a  clump  of  rhododendrons  one-half  mile 
away.  In  this  connection  another  observation  is  of  interest.  An  apiarist  at 
McBride,  B.C.,  was  the  only  one  keeping  bees  in  the  district.  Bees  from  this 
apiary  were  noted  working  in  a  field  of  sweet  clover  four  miles  from  the  apiary, 
though  an  abundance  of  alsike  and  Dutch  clover  was  available  in  the  intervening 
territory.  In  districts  in  which  little  but  apple  bloom  was  present,  the  same 
number  of  colonies  should  obviously  give  a  greater  field  force  for  apple  pollina- 
tion than  in  districts  in  which  a  variety  of  attractive  bloom  was  present.  Apple 
is  certainly  not  the  most-favoured  plant;  dandelions,  rhododendrons  and  other 
plants  available  during  apple  bloom  being  apparently  preferred  under  ordinary 


151 

conditions.     Hence,  the  more  of  such  bloom  in  a  given  locality,  the  less  bees 
would  there  be  available  for  apple  pollination. 

(vi)  Physical  Features. — The  presence  of  hills,  ravines,  strips  of  woods, 
etc.,  frequently  affect  the  flight  and  concentration  of  bees  used  for  pollination 
purposes.  One  apiary,  situated  at  the  north  of  a  large  orchard,  has  a  steep 
bank  at  the  rear.  The  result  is  that  the  greatest  numbers  of  the  field  force  from 
this  apiary  fly  due  north  across  a  meadow  and  a  stream  and  work  on  the  apple 
bloom  in  the  orchards  about  a  mile  away.  It  is  difficult  to  predict  how  bees 
will  work  in  broken  territory,  but  the  influence  of  factors  similar  to  those  men- 
tioned above,  must  be  taken  into  account  in  deciding  how  bees  are  to  be  placed 
in  the  orchard. 

(vii)  Weather  Conditions. — The  effect  of  weather  conditions  on  flight  and 
concentration  have  been  discussed  at  length  elsewhere.  Inasmuch  as  unfavour- 
able weather  limits  the  length  and  duration  of  flights,  it  is  obvious  that  the 
hives  should  be  so  distributed  about  the  orchard  as  to  ensure  proper  pollination 
of  all  trees  in  times  of  unfavourable  weather.  Observations,  made  in  1932 
particularly,  substantiate  the  observations  of  other  workers  that,  at  such  periods, 
there  is  greater  activity  near  the  hives ;  and  that  in  brief  periods  of  fine  weather 
there  is  a  tendency  to  restrict  flight  and  activity  to  points  not  far  distant  from 
the  colonies. 

(viii)  Area  of  Activity. — A  factor  that  deserves  consideration  at  this  point 
is  the  habit  of  bees  of  working  in  a  limited  locality.  It  is  the  testimony  of  a 
number  of  careful  students  that  pollen-  or  nectar-gathering  over  an  area  is  not 
indiscriminate ;  but  that  bees  tend  to  go  to  the  same  place  and  return  to  it,  trip 
after  trip,  until  supplies  are  exhausted.  The  fact  that  pollination  is  usually 
heaviest  on  the  side  of  a  tree  next  the  pollinizer  and  that  the  effect  is  lost  a 
very  few  rows  away  has  been  often  noted. 

Minderhoud  (1931)  reviews  the  observations  of  other  workers  in  this  field 
and  presents  his  own  experiments  with  marked  bees  on  the  flowers  of  Taraxacum 
officinale  Weber,  Trijolium  sp.,  Cruciferce,  etc.  He  considers  that  his  data 
warrant  the  following  conclusions:  In  visiting  low  growing  plants  and  in  the 
absence  of  a  strong  wind  the  honey  bee  will  for  a  long  time  visit  one  particular 
place,  the  area  of  which  does  not  ordinarily  exceed  10  by  10  metres.  Under 
such  circumstances,  its  successive  visits  are  to  flowers  not  farther  apart  gener- 
ally, than  one  metre.  He  concludes  that  one  can  only  expect  to  get  full  benefit 
from  bees,  when  different  pollens  are  available  to  them,  within  a  small  radius. 
Why  this  should  be  so  is  suggested  by  studies  by  Von  Frisch  (1924),  which 
indicate  that  odour  from  the  scent  glands  of  the  workers  of  a  given  colony, 
impregnating  the  spot  visited,  acts  as  a  lure  to  other  workers  from  the  same 
colony,  but  not  for  workers  from  another  hive.  Bonnier  (1906),  for  example, 
marked  all  the  bees  he  found  visiting  a  five-metre  strip  of  buckwheat,  and  the 
next  day  found  only  marked  bees  visiting  the  strip. 

On  several  occasions  during  our  experiments,  certain  colonies  have  shown 
very  severe  poisoning,  while  others  have  shown  little  or  none.  A  good  example 
of  this  was  in  1929,  when  no  severe  poisoning  could  be  detected  in  49  colonies 
placed  in  an  orchard,  but  one  colony  showed  poisoning  of  the  most  severe  type. 
It  seems  difficult  to  explain  this  fact  other  than  on  the  assumption  that  the 
bees  from  this  particular  colony  secured  poison  from  a  common  source.  As  a 
matter  of  fact,  a  small  plot  not  far  from  this  hive  had  been  dusted  with  sulphur 
lead  arsenate  dust  which  may  have  furnished  the  source  of  the  poisoning. 
Similar  evidence,  with  respect  to  the  gathering  of  certain  kinds  of  nectar  bv 
particular  hives,  will  occur  to  any  experienced  beekeeper. 

(ix)  Amount  of  Bloom  Available  in  Relation  to  Population  Present.— The 
amount  of  bee  pasturage  available  in  a  given  area,  relative  to  the  population 


152 

present,  has  a  very  important  bearing  on  the  distance  of  flight  and  the  point 
of  maximum  concentration.  Thus,  in  a  territory  with  small  scattered  orchards 
and  little  other  bloom  available,  we  would  expect  them  to  go  farther  afield 
than  in  a  densely  orcharded  area  during  the  period  of  bloom.  Evidence  is 
available  from  Fort  Vermilion,  Alberta,  vouched  for  by  the  Superintendent  of 
the  Experimental  Station  and  a  Hudson  Bay  Company  official  at  the  place, 
that  bees  from  the  Experimental  Station,  the  only  hive  bees  in  the  entire  area, 
were  taken  seven  miles  away  at  the  Hudson  Bay  Company  office.  This,  how- 
ever, represents  a  condition  quite  different  from  that  in  the  territory  in  which 
our  studies  were  made,  where  large  blocks  of  orchard  were  everywhere  present. 
Our  counts  indicate  that,  up  to  a  certain  point,  when  the  apiary  strength  is 
increased,  the  bees  range  farther  afield,  as  was  the  case  in  Somerset  in  1932, 
when  50  strong  colonies  were  placed  in  an  orchard  with  44J  acres  of  orchard 
within  a  quarter-mile  radius  and  137  acres  within  a  half-mile  radius.  Here,  bees 
were  readily  detected  in  appreciable  numbers  a  mile  away.  Flight  and  concen- 
tration are,  therefore,  affected  by  the  number  and  strength  of  the  individual 
colonies  and  by  the  number  of  colonies  available  in  any  one  place.  This  fact 
is  important  when  considering  the  number  of  colonies  required  to  secure  pollina- 
tion in  any  given  fruit  growing  area. 

Various  recommendations  are  made  as  to  the  number  of  colonies  per  acre 
required  to  ensure  adequate  pollination  of  apple  orchards.  The  majority  of 
these  vary,  from  one  colony  to  each  four  acres  of  young  bearing  orchards,  to 
one  colony  per  acre  for  large  trees.  A  few  typical  references  at  this  point  will 
suffice  for  our  purpose. 

Hooper  (1929)  recommends  one  hive  of  bees  to  each  acre  of  fruit  trees, 
especially  in  a  district  unsuitable  for  wild  bees.  DeOng  (1925)  considers  one 
strong  hive  to  the  acre  as  a  sufficient  pollinating  force.  Murneek  (1930)  believes 
that  one  colony  to  every  three  or  five  acres  will  be  sufficient  for  a  young  orchard 
that  has  just  come  into  bearing,  but  that  older  orchards  mayNneed  a  hive  per 
acre.  It  is  contended  by  Philp  and  Vansell  (1932)  that  the  usual  recommenda- 
tion of  one  hive  per  acre  is  more  than  necessary  under  some  conditions,  but 
weather  conditions  so  affect  results  that  more  would  often  be  justified. 

Certain  recommendations  would  seem  to  assume  either  that  bees  stay  in 
the  orchards  in  which  they  are  placed,  or  else  that  all  the  orchards  in  the 
immediate  neighbourhood  will  have  colonies  at  an  equal  rate.  If  we  assume 
that  hives  with  an  average  field  force  of  15,000  bees  are  placed  at  the  rate  of 
one  to  the  acre  in  a  ten-acre  orchard  of  large  bearing  trees  with  contiguous 
orchards  surrounding  it,  aggregating  125  acres  and  that,  under  these  conditions, 
the  bees  spread  out  only  in  a  quarter-mile  radius  from  the  colonies  and  also 
assuming  for  the  purpose  of  illustration  that  all  trees  are  in  bloom  at  once, 
there  should  be  present  one  bee  for  each  833  blossoms.  If,  however,  we  assume 
that  the  total  field  force  from  the  ten  colonies  was  evenly  distributed  over 
the  ten  acres,  the  figure  would  be  one  bee  for  each  66  blossoms.  It  is,  of  course, 
realized  that  all  blossoms  are  not  out  at  once,  that  the  distribution  is  not  even 
and  that  the  bees  may  fly  considerably  farther  than  one  quarter  of  a  mile, 
but  the  illustration  will  serve  to  stress  the  point  that  the  bee  population 
of  the  surrounding  area  is  an  important  factor  in  the  utilization  of  bees  in 
orchards.  The  situation  is  quite  different,  in  cases  where  contiguous  orchards 
are  similarly  supplied,  from  that  created  by  the  entire  absence  of  bees  in  the 
surrounding  area.  This  fact  was  repeatedly  observed  during  the  course  oi  our 
studies. 

The  writer,  on  one  occasion  in  1930,  counted  for  two  entire  days  in  a  ten- 
acre  orchard  in  which  ten  strong   colonies  were  placed,   without   observing   a 
single  bee  working  the  bloom,  conditions  for  flight  and  bee  activity  being  i  * 
lent.    Within  a  one-quarter  mile  radius  there  was  a  total  of  about  90  acres  of 


153 

orchard  spread  over  approximately  200  acres  of  land.  The  point  selected  was 
225  yards  from  the  colonies.  Six  counters,  placed  in  the  orchard  at  intervals  of 
from  3  to  225  yards  from  the  hives,  secured  only  an  average  of  0-37  bees  per 
10-minute  count  during  the  same  period.  In  1931,  forty  strong  colonies  were 
placed  in  the  same  orchard  equally  divided  between  the  two  ends  of  the  orchard, 
the  average  counts  giving  a  total  of  0-62  per  10-minute  count  under  very 
similar  conditions.  It  was  thus  necessary  to  use  four  times  as  many  colonies 
to  get  less  than  double  the  population  in  the  bloom.  Similar,  or  even  more 
pronounced  results  were  obtained  elsewhere  when  colonies  were  placed  one  to 
the  acre.  The  entire  area  in  which  our  work  has  been  done  is  heavily  covered 
with  orchard  and  at  most  stations  the  number  of  bees  determined  by  counts 
was  negligible.  At  Blomidon,  in  a  fourteen-acre  orchard  with  one  colony  per 
acre,  we  obtained,  in  1930,  only  an  average  of  -22  bees  per  count.  In  this  case 
the  bees  were  placed  at  the  north  end  of  the  orchard,  with  a  mountain  at  the 
back  covered  by  forest,  while  south  and  east  of  the  colonies,  stretched  a  solid 
block  of  orchard  surrounding  the  one  in  which  the  observations  were  made. 

On  the  other  hand,  on  Long  island  where  we  had  approximately  90  acres 
geographically  isolated  from  other  orchards  and  situated  on  a  total  land  area 
of  640  acres,  and  with  colonies  present  at  the  same  rate,  we  had  an  average 
of  1-23  bees  per  count  in  1931.  In  1932,  with  stronger  colonies  and  not  more 
than  60  per  cent  of  the  bloom,  our  average  count  was  3-05  bees.  Thus,  on 
Long  island,  with  one  colony  per  acre  of  orchard,  there  were  many  more  bees 
available  for  pollination  than  at  Lakeville  with  four  colonies  per  acre,  since  in 
the  latter  location  the  contiguous  orchard  area  was  unprovided  with  bees.  In 
1932,  immediately  before  using  them  on  Long  island,  50  strong  colonies  were 
placed  in  an  orchard  at  Somerset  where  92  acres  of  orchard,  within  a  half-mile 
radius,  were  at  their  maximum  bloom.  Here,  in  the  orchard  immediately  con- 
tiguous to  the  hive,  we  secured  an  average  count  of  5-53  bees.  This  average 
is  based  on  a  2-day  count  of  optimum  flight,  whereas  the  Long  island  count 
is  based  on  the  period  of  bloom.  The  foregoing  clearly  indicates  that,  in  making 
recommendations  for  placing  hive  bees  in  an  orchard,  it  is  necessary  to  consider 
not  only  the  acreage  of  the  particular  orchard  in  which"  the  bees  are  placed,  but 
also  the  area  of  surrounding  orchards  and  whether  or  not  they  are  provided  with 
bees.  In  other  words,  the  district  and  not  the  orchard  must  be  considered  the 
unit  for  calculating  the  force  necessary. 

Our  observations  indicate  that  there  must  be  a  certain  minimum  popula- 
tion present  under  such  conditions  before  a  sufficient  concentration  of  bees,  in 
the  particular  orchard  in  which  they  are  placed,  can  be  assured.  This  minimum 
on  the  basis  of  our  observation  is  about  fifty  colonies  in  an  area  solidly  planted 
to  fruit  trees.  In  other  words,  if  ten  colonies  are  used  to  pollinate  an  orchard 
of  ten  acres,  surrounded  by  90  acres  without  bees,  this  would  not  necessarily 
ensure  the  pollination  of  that  particular  orchard  which  would  require  in  the 
neighborhood  of  fifty  colonies  in  order  to  ensure  results. 

With  a  population  sufficient  to  give  an  average  of  one  bee  per  10-minute 
count,  it  has  been  estimated  that  each  bee  would  be  required  to  pollinate  250 
blossoms  in  order  that  every  blossom  in  the  orchard,  in  a  year  of  maximum  bloom, 
should  be  visited.  Coming  again  to  Long  island  where  conditions  were  more 
under  control,  we  find  that  the  counts  taken  over  the  whole  island  in  1931,  a 
year  of  maximum  bloom,  amounted  to  1-27  per  count  during  several  days  of 
bloom  and  in  1932,  with  approximately  60  per  cent  of  the  previous  year's  bloom, 
3-05  per  count.  Placing  the  lowest  possible  estimate  on  the  number  of  blossoms 
that  a  colony  of  bees  is  capable  of  visiting,  it  would  appear  that  one  colony 
per  acre,  under  conditions  of  isolation,  would  represent  a  population  much 
greater  than  the  number  theoretically  necessary.  If  our  record  of  3-05  bees 
per  count,  representing  the  force  liberated  by  a  strong  colony  with  a  field  force 
of  15,000  bees  or  upward,  is  representative,  even  supposing  that  only  25  to  50 


154 

per  cent  were  effective  pollinators,  it  is  obvious  that  each  bee  would  have 
to  visit  only  a  comparatively  small  number  of  blossoms  in  order  to  pollinate  the 
whole  orchard.  In  considering  this  problem,  allowance  must  be  made  for  dupli- 
cate visits,  proportion  of  nectar  gatherers  to  pollen  gatherers,  etc.  Though,  in 
order  to  ensure  pollination  under  unfavourable  conditions,  it  is  necessary  to 
supply  a  much  larger  force  than  is  theoretically  necessary,  it  would  appear  that 
one  hive  to  the  acre  of  bearing  trees  should  be  quite  adequate,  even  after  all 
allowances  are  made. 

In  recommending  a  certain  force  per  acre  it  should  be  emphasized  that 
such  recommendations  only  apply,  in  the  case  of  areas  heavily  planted  to 
orchard,  where  neighbouring  orchards  are  similarly  supplied.  Colonies,  set  out 
at  this  rate  in  small  blocks  of  orchard  surrounded  by  hundreds  of  acres  destitute 
of  bees,  spread  out  so  thinly  as  to  make  them  of  doubtful  value  for  pollination 
purposes.  This  point  is  particularly  emphasized,  because  many  growers  in  dis- 
tricts where  no  hive  bees  at  all  are  present,  consider  it  sufficient  to  use  only  a 
very  few  colonies,  sometimes  no  more  than  one  or  two  in  a  large  orchard. 

(x)  Method  of  Placing. — Most  studies  that  have  been  made  with  respect 
to  the  method  of  placing  colonies  in  or  about  orchards,  have  had  to  do  mainly 
with  the  problem  of  the  best  method  of  placement  to  follow  in  order  to  secure 
uniform  distribution.  There  appears  to  be  a  wide  difference  of  opinion  between 
different  workers  as  to  the  proper  method  of  distributing  the  colonies. 

Data  secured  by  Hutson  (1926)  were  said  to  indicate  that  the  bees  worked 
most  freely  near  the  hives  and  that  by  placing  hives  singly  210  feet  apart 
each  way  and  15  feet  from  pollinizing  bouquets,  maximum  results  were  secured 
and  there  was  a  uniform  distribution  of  bees  in  the  orchard.  Counts  show  con- 
centration about  hives  placed  in  groups,  and  the  numbers  diminish  as  distance 
from  the  hive  group  increases.  Haseman  (1932)  states  that  colonies  should  be 
scattered  through  the  orchard  since,  in  cool  weather,  bees  will  not  fly  far. 
Marshall,  Johnson,  et  at.  (1929)  emphasize  the  point  that  colonies  should  be 
placed  near  effective  pollinizers.  Murneek  (1930)  recommends  that  the  hives 
should  be  distributed  throughout  the  orchard  and  not  kept  in  one  sheltered 
corner.  Within  a  heavily  bearing  orchard  the  greatest  service  will  be  secured 
when  the  colonies  are  placed  200  feet  apart. 

Philp  and  Vansell  (1931)  have  a  different  idea  as  to  the  proper  placing  of 
colonies.  They  state  that  under  ordinary  conditions  bees  should  be  left  in 
orchards  in  groups  of  10  colonies.  They  point  out  that  sunshine,  wind,  temper- 
ature, rain,  and  other  factors  outside  the  hive,  affect  the  flight  of  bees  and  there- 
fore general  recommendations  regarding  placement  are  useless.  If  the  weather 
is  fair,  bees  can  fly  far  enough  to  cover  100  acres  of  orchard,  or  more,  from  one 
location;  but  in  cold  weather  flight  is  limited.  They  state  that,  under  average 
conditions,  bees  in  groups  of  10  to  20  colonies,  for  as  many  acres  surrounding 
them,  have  been  found  very  satisfactory.  Accessible  situations  should  be  chosen 
to  facilitate  placement  and  removal. 

It  was  determined  in  our  tests  that,  in  a  densely  orcharded  area  supplied 
with  colonies  in  groups,  but  at  the  rate  of  one  per  acre,  little  consistent  differ- 
ence occurred  up  to  about  660  feet  from  the  colonies;  but  after  double  this  dis- 
tance had  been  reached  counts  became  definitely  lower  and  beyond  one  quarter 
of  a  mile  few  bees  were  found.  Since  it  is  sometimes  more  convenient,  both  in 
placing  and  removing  colonies,  to  distribute  them  in  groups  rather  than  singly 
throughout  the  orchard,  it  would  appear  to  be  satisfactory  to  have  the  colonies 
placed  in  groups  one-quarter  mile  apart,  having  regard  to  the  contour  of  the 
land  and  the  other  factors  already  discussed. 

After  experimenting  for  three  seasons  on  methods  of  distributing  colonies 
without  being  able  to  distinguish  any  great  difference  between  colonies  distributed 
singly  and  in  groups,  we  arranged  to  carry  out  this  latter  plan  on  Long  island 


155 


in  1932.  The  island  being  2  miles  long,  the  extreme  colonies  were  placed  approx- 
imately one-quarter  of  a  mile  from  each  end,  and  the  remainder  at  approximately 
one-quarter  mile  intervals,  there  being  3  apiaries  of  15  colonies  and  3  apiaries  of 
16  colonies,  or  approximately  one  per  acre  of  orchard.  As  there  was  a  different 
number  of  counts  on  certain  of  the  days,  the  data  for  June  2,  June  3  and  for  the 
average  count  for  June  4,  5,  and  9  taken  together,  are  presented.  Counters  were 
placed  at  the  extreme  ends  of  the  island  and  midway  between  each  of  the  apiaries, 
also  at  the  two  extreme  apiaries  and  the  centre  apiary.  While  the  error  due  to 
the  human  element  in  these  counts  may  be  large,  it  was  apparent  that  the  greatest 
number  of  bees  was  rarely  present  in  the  orchard  adjacent  to  the  apiary,  and  at 
all  times  the  large  number  present  at  station  "  Y  "  was  noticed  by  all  observers. 
It  would  not  appear,  therefore,  that  the  placing  of  colonies  singly  and  evenly 
through  the  orchards  could  ensure  an  even  distribution  of  bees,  when  the  foregoing 
facts  are  considered.  It  would  seem  to  be  equally  effective  to  place  the  colonies 
in  groups  at  suitable  intervals,  with  due  regard  to  the  position  of  orchards  and 
the  surrounding  physical  features,  as  good,  or  even  better,  distribution  was 
obtained  by  placing  groups  of  colonies  at  one-quarter  mile  intervals  as  by 
placing  them  singly  in  the  orchard,  though  nothing  like  perfect  distribution  was 
ever  obtained  by  either  method.  The  habit  of  the  bees  of  working  nearer  the 
hives  during  unfavourable  conditions  would  render  it  inadvisable  to  place  the 
colonies  at  greater  intervals  than  that  indicated.  The  accompanying  table  shows 
the  results  of  counts  made  at  the  various  stations  on  Long  island. 

TABLE  27.— AVERAGE  COUNTS  OF  BEES  FROM  DIFFERENT  STATIONS  ON  LONG  ISLAND,  1932 


Average  for  June  2 

Average  for  June  3 

Average  for  June  4 

5  and  9 

Station 

Hive 
bees 

Wild 
bees 

Number 
of  obser- 
vations 

Hive 
bees 

Wild 
bees 

Number 
of  obser- 
vations 

Hive 

bees 

Wild 
bees 

Number 
of  obser- 
vations 

Z          

9-09 
4-78 

13-85 
9-35 
6-54 
0 
0-33 
7-25 

10-90 
8-28 
5-93 

1-34 
3-OG 
7-27 
2-39 
2-85 
0-29 
0-5 
5-71 
2-56 
1-27 
1  61 
* 

41 
41 
41 
41 
13 
7 
6 
41 
41 
41 
41 

0-59 
0-88 
0-76 
0-61 
0-22 
0-08 
0 
0-45 
0-59 
0-26 
0-82 
2-76 

0-85 
0-78 
1-93 
0-29 
0-30 
0-08 
0 
2-64 
0-66 
0-18 
0-62 
0-92 

85 

1                      

85 

A..                   

1-46 
2-62 
5-54 

0 
0-31 
0-08 

13 
13 
13 

85 

B...                 

85 

C 

27 

12 

12 

4                                 

85 

D                                 

16-75 

4-61 

015 
0 

13 
13 

85 

E                              

85 

6 

85 

Y....                

* 

* 

* 

85 

*No  regular  count  on  these  dates  but  observations  indicated  bees  to  be  abundant. 

Where  the  apiary  is  situated  near  the  orchard  and  permanent  in  position,  it 
should  be  located  with  reference  to  the  contour  of  the  land  so  that  the  maximum 
number  of  bees  will  work  in  the  orchard  and  not  fly  out  of  it.  Steep  banks  in  the 
neighbourhood  of  the  orchard,  or  intervening  belts  of  trees  may,  deflect  the  usual 
line  of  flight  as  already  explained.  With  the  apiary  in  a  permanent  location 
near  the  orchard  the  distribution  of  colonies  throughout  the  orchard  occasions 
loss.  At  the  Experimental  Station,  Kentville,  it  was  found  that  the  colonies 
were  much  weakened  by  bees  returning  to  the  original  stand,  though  a  few  weak 
colonies  left  behind  to  catch  "  drifters  "  was  of  benefit.  When  the  colonies  were 
returned  to  the  original  stand,  bees  that  had  emerged  during  the  interval  spent 
in  the  orchard,  returned  to  the  spot  where  their  hive  had  rested,  clustering  on 
the  spot  for  days,  stinging  all  who  approached.  On  the  other  hand,  no  such 
trouble  was  experienced  from  two  apiaries  each  distant  about  a  mile  from  the 
original  stand. 

(d)  TIME   OF   PLACING 

As  regards  time  of  placing,  most  workers  state  that  best  results  can  be 
expected  when  the  bees  are  placed  in  the  orchard  only  when  the  trees  come  into 
bloom  and  removed  when  the  majority  of  the  petals  have  fallen.     There  is  an 


156 

added  reason  for  doing  this  when  danger  of  poisoning  is  to  be  anticipated.  Our 
experience  has  been  that  this  danger  is  materially  lessened,  though  not  necessarily 
eliminated,  if  the  practice  outlined  above  is  followed,  and  it  appears  to  be  a 
sound  one  from  every  point  of  view. 

(e)  SOURCE  OF  POLLEN  SUPPLY 

Bees  are  useless  in  the  orchard  without  a  suitable  pollen  supply,  a  fact 
which  appears  to  be  often  overlooked  by  growers.  In  fact  it  is  quite  conceivable 
that  some  harm  may  result.  Bees  placed  in  a  block  of  self-unfruitful  varieties 
may  only  result  in  rapidly  selfing  that  variety,  thus  inhibiting  a  certain  amount 
of  crossing  that  might  occur  through  the  agency  of  wild  species  of  bees  before 
the  period  of  stigma  receptivity  has  passed.  On  the  other  hand,  if  cross-unfruit- 
ful sorts  are  mixed  together,  the  pollination  of  the  varieties  concerned  with  the 
pollen  of  the  other,  may  result  in  even  less  fruit  than  when  certain  varieties  are 
selfed.  Therefore,  the  proper  provision  and  distribution  of  effective  pollinizing 
varieties  of  apples  is  equally  important  to  that  of  the  insect  pollinators  and  is 
something  that  is  under  the  control  of  the  owner  who  sets  out  the  orchard. 

The  problems  connected  with  the  foregoing,  however,  have  been  discussed 
in  detail  elsewhere.  In  our  work  we  have  found  the  question  of  providing  a 
suitable  pollen  supply  of  more  practical  importance  than  the  provision  of  bees 
alone,  and  even  using  "  bouquets  "  of  fruitful  varieties  in  connection  with  bees 
has  usually  been  disappointing. 

3.    SECURING  COLONIES 

Three  methods  of  using  bees  are  suggested  by  DeOng  (1925),  as  follows:  — 

1.  Rental  from  a  professional  beekeeper. 

2.  Employment  of  a  trained  beekeeper  to  care  for  bees  owned  or  leased 

by  one  or  a  group  of  orchardistv 

3.  Ownership  and  personal  care. 

The  two  first  methods  are  considered  best,  as  the  orchardist  usually  has 
neither  time  nor  knowledge  to  care  for  the  bees  properly.  The  difficulties  of 
the  average  fruit  grower  are  increased,  under  Nova  Scotia  conditions,  on  account 
of  the  evidence  of  poisoning,  which  may  cause  the  beekeeper,  and  particularly 
the  inexperienced  beekeeper,  to  lose  all  his  colonies,  or  to  have  them  become 
so  weakened  as  to  be  worthless  for  pollination  purposes. 

At  the  present  time  there  is  no  one  in  the  business  of  renting  colonies  to 
beekeepers  in  this  province  and  there  are  not  available  a  sufficient  number  of 
colonies  to  supply  any  real  demand.  Packages  can  be  obtained  from  the  South 
at  the  rate  of  from  $1.75  to  $2.75  for  2-pound  packages,  depending  upon  the 
source  and  number  purchased  at  one  time.  Similarly,  3-pound  packages  will 
cost  from  $2.25  to  $3.50;  4-pound  packages  from  $2.75  to  $4.25,  and  5-pound 
packages  from  $3.50  to  $5  each.  Then,  transportation  charges  will  have  to  be 
added  to  the  foregoing  prices,  and  with  proper  care  and  attention,  these  pack- 
ages can,  if  purchased  sufficiently  early,  be  put  in  condition  to  be  of  some 
service  during  bloom.  The  price  asked  by  commercial  beekeepers  for  strong 
overwintered  colonies  varies  considerably  in  different  fruit  growing  districts. 
As  already  stated,  renting  bees  has  never  been  practised  in  Nova  Scotia,  except 
in  a  few  isolated  areas,  and  accordingly  few  figure-  are  available,  but  the  risk 
of  poisoning  would  probably  force  a  maximum  price. 

Farrar  (1929)  states  that  in  Massachusetts,  beekeepers  feel  that  they  should 
receive  from  $5  to  $10  per  colony.  This  writer  suggests  s;>  ;is  a  fair  price  for 
a  colony  covering  5-6  frames,  deducting  $1.50  for  each  frame  less,  and  adding  $1 
for  each  frame  in  excess  of  5  or  6  combs.  Marshall,  et  al.  (1929).  in  Michigan, 
states  that  $2.50  to  $3  per  colony  is  the  regular  price  in  that  state.  Phillips  (1930) 
claims  that  labour  and  other  inconveniences  bring  the  cost  to  the  beekeeper  to  at 
least  $5,  but,  at  this  price,  beekeepers  find  that  they  cannot  do  the  work  or  take 


157 

the  necessary  risk.  He  states  that  as  high  as  $10  per  colony  has  been  charged 
for  rent.  Philp  and  Vansell  (1932),  in  California,  emphasize  the  fact  that  the 
rental  charged  will  depend  upon  distance  of  moving,  state  of  roads,  rainfall,  supply 
and  demand,  etc.,  but  that  at  the  present  time,  $2  will  represent  about  the  average 
price.  Another  possible  method  that  has  been  suggested  is  for  a  group  of 
orchardists  to  co-operate  in  securing  colonies  and  engaging  a  beekeeper  to  tend 
them. 

4.   GENERAL   CONCLUSIONS 

While  various  causes  prevented  the  complete  carrying  out  of  our  studies  as 
originally  planned,  the  following  conclusions  seem  justified  on  the  basis  of  our 
investigations: 

(a)  It  is  generally  recognized  that  the  provision  of  hive  bees  to  ensure 
pollination  in  apple  orchards  is  a  sound  practice;  but  the  experimental  evidence 
in  its  favour  is  not  so  clear-cut  as  in  the  case  of  certain  other  fruits.  In  certain 
seasons,  in  some  localities  at  least,  adequate  pollination  is  effected  by  native 
solitary  bees. 

(b)  Where  this  condition  does  not  obtain  the  provision  of  a  suitable  number 
of  colonies  should  be  practised  as  a  part  of  the  regular  orchard  routine  and. 
even  in  cases  where  solitary  bees  may  be  adequate  for  pollination  in  favourable 
years,  the  placing  of  colonies  in  the  orchard  may  be  regarded  as  a  wise  insurance 
against  seasons  when  conditions  are  unfavourable. 

(c)  Experimental  evidence  regarding  the  value  of  bees  for  pollination  pur- 
poses under  controlled  conditions  demonstrates  clearly  the  necessity  of  bees 
combined  with  a  supply  of  suitable  pollen  for  all  varieties.  Even  the  most  self- 
fruitful  varieties  require  bees  in  order  to  ensure  adequate  pollination. 

(d)  Many  factors,  including  colony  strength  and  number  of  colonies  in 
relation  to  the  adjacent  orchard  area,  the  physical  features  of  the  area  con- 
cerned, weather  conditions  and  many  others  that  have  been  discussed  in  detail 
in  the  foregoing  pages,  influence  the  flight,  concentration  and  distribution  of  bees. 

(e)  The  point  is  particularly  emphasized  that,  in  estimating  the  number 
of  colonies  per  acre  of  orchard,  the  surrounding  district  and  not  the  individual 
orchard  should  be  considered  the  unit.  The  placing  of  a  few  colonies  in  an 
orchard  area  devoid  of  bees  does  not  necessarily  ensure  the  pollination  of  the 
particular  orchard  in  which  they  are  placed,  since  they  may  work  mainly 
in  another  orchard  or  spread  out  too  thinly  over  surrounding  orchards  to  be  of 
great  value.  The  recommendation  of  one  to  the  acre  or  one  to  four  acres  has 
little  meaning,  unless  it  is  known  that  the  surrounding  area  is  similarly  pro- 
vided. Under  conditions  of  isolation,  or  where  neighbouring  orchards  are  also 
provided  with  bees,  one  colony  to  an  acre  of  mature  trees  should  make  adequate 
provision  for  pollination,  even  allowing  for  considerable  unfavourable  weather. 

(/)  Bees  should  be  placed  throughout  the  orchard  area  in  such  a  way  as 
to  ensure  as  good  distribution  as  possible  and  thus  take  advantage  of  short 
flights  that  may  occur  at  intervals  between  periods  of  bad  weather.  Placing 
colonies  at  the  rate  per  acre  indicated,  but  in  equidistant  groups  of  from  5  to 
15,  depending  upon  the  area  in  orchard,  is  regarded  as  satisfactory.  Further 
details  are  given  in  previous  pages. 

(g)  It  is  best  to  place  the  bees  in  the  orchards  when  the  earliest  varieties  are 
in  full  bloom  and  remove  them  before  the  petals  have  fallen  from  the  later 
varieties. 

(h)  The  proper  distribution  of  cross-fruitful  varieties  in  an  orchard  is  a 
primary  requisite  to  successfull  pollination;  without  such  provision,  placing  bees 
in  the  orchard  only  results  in  unfruitful  selling  or  crossing. 

(i)  Colonies  may  be  rented  from  a  beekeeper  or  they  may  be  owned  and 
cared  for  by  the  orchardist;  but  unless  the  latter  is  skilled  in  the  care  of  bees, 
the  former  method  is  usually  preferable.  Another  method  suggested  is  for  a 
group  of  orchardists  to  co-operate  and  hire  a  skilled  beekeeper. 


V.  STUDIES  IN  BEE  POISONING  AS  A  PHASE  OF  THE 
ORCHARD  POLLINATION  STUDIES 

By  F.  A.  Herman  and  W.  H.  Brittain 

A.  INTRODUCTION 

Owing  to  the  importance  of  the  evidence  of  poisoning  among  bees,  in  any 
consideration  of  their  use  for  pollination  purposes,  the  following  studies  were 
undertaken  as  an  integral  part  of  a  pollination  project  undertaken  by  the 
Dominion  Department  of  Agriculture  in  the  Annapolis  valley,  N.S.,  during  the 
years  1928-1932  inclusive. 

In  carrying  out  this  work  the  writers  have  had  invaluable  assistance  from 
Mr.  C.  B.  Gooderham,  Dominion  Apiarist,  in  the  way  of  active  assistance  and 
advice.  A  number  of  the  photographs  used  in  this  report  were  also  furnished 
by  him.  Mr.  Evan  Craig,  Apiarist  at  the  Experimental  Station,  Kentville,  has 
given  freely  of  his  time,  not  only  in  caring  for  the  bees  used  in  the  work,  but 
also  in  helping  with  the  actual  work  of  the  investigation.  Mr.  H.  G.  Payne, 
Provincial  Apiarist,  Truro,  took  an  active  part  in  the  field  studies  and  was  of 
great  assistance  through  his  local  knowledge  of  the  industry. 

B.  HISTORICAL 

Ever  since  the  dawn  of  modern  spraying  practices,  complaints  of  losses  of 
bees  from  arsenical  poisoning  have  been  heard  with  increasing  frequency,  and 
laws  prohibiting  the  applications  of  sprays  during  bloom  have  been  passed  by 
several  states  and  provinces. 

A  number  of  chemists  and  entomologists  have  given  the  matter  attention, 
and,  while  a  complete  review  of  the  literature  would  be  out  of  the  question  in 
the  space  available,  a  few  of  the  more  important  contributions  may  serve  to 
give  a  picture  of  the  present  state  of  knowledge  with  respect  to  this  problem. 
A  more  complete  review  has  been  given  by  Mclndoo  and  Demuth  (1926). 

Webster  (1896)  was  one  of  the  earliest  workers  to  conduct  definite  experi- 
ments in  connection  with  this  problem.  Paris  green,  4  ounces  to  50  gallons 
of  water,  was  sprayed  on  a  Lombard  plum  tree  in  full  bloom  at  2  p.m.,  the 
quantity  of  the  mixture  applied  being  just  sufficient  to  wet  thoroughly  without 
dripping.  The  tree  was  enclosed  in  canvas  and  mosquito  netting;  5^  hours 
after  spraying  a  hive  of  bees  which  had  been  placed  near  the  tree  some  two 
wreeks  previously,  was  moved  into  the  tent  and  the  bees  liberated.  The  follow- 
ing morning  many  dead  and  dying  bees  were  found  on  the  tent  floor.  The  dead 
bees  were  examined  by  the  Marsh  method  for  arsenic,  and  yielded  positive 
results.  Further  tests  from  washed  bee-  were  also  positive.  Several  days  later 
many  more  dead  bees  were  picked  up,  and  after  washing,  first  with  water,  then 
with  a  weak  solution  of  ammonia,  were  analysed  and  showed  the  presence  of 
arsenic.  A  small  apple  orchard,  from  which  the  bloom  had  all  fallen  except  a 
belated  cluster,  was  sprayed  with  Bordeaux  mixture,  to  which  had  been  added 
Paris  green  at  the  rate  of  4  ounces  to  each  50  gallons.  A  few  days  alter  this 
application,  three  previously  strong  colonies  of  bees  located  nearby  suddenly 
became  depopulated.  Analysis  of  dead  bees  and  dead  brood  showed  the  presence 
of  arsenic.  Conclusions  drawn  from  these  experiments  are  as  follows:  Bees 
are  liable  to  be  poisoned  by  spraying  the  bloom  oi   fruit   trees,  the   liability 

158 


159 

increasing  in  proportion  as  the  weather  is  favourable  for  the  activity  of  the 
bees,  and  all  the  bloom  must  have  fallen  from  the  trees  before  the  danger  will 
have  ceased. 

Price  (1920)  conducted  laboratory  and  field  work  to  find  out  the  amount 
of  soluble  arsenic  necessary  to  kill  a  bee,  to  find  out  if  a  bee  working  upon  a 
mixture  of  insoluble  arsenic  and  syrup  would  take  up  the  arsenic  particles,  and, 
if  the  dead  bees  contained  arsenic  internally.  A  small  amount  of  arsenic  of 
less  than  -0000005  grams  of  As205  is  a  fatal  dose  for  a  bee  and  the  longevity 
of  bees  poisoned  with  arsenic  was  found  to  depend  upon  the  size  of  the  dose. 
It  was  also  found  that  a  bee  takes  up  lead  arsenate  particles  in  feeding  on  a 
sugar  solution  having  them  in  suspension.  An  examination  of  dead  bees  from 
laboratory  or  field  experiments  showed  arsenic  to  be  present  both  internally 
and  externally.  It  was  found  that  bees  worked  freely  on  sprayed  trees  in  the 
open,  even  when  there  were  unsprayed  trees  all  about  and  that  trees  sprayed 
while  in  blossom  contained  sufficient  poison  to  cause  a  tremendous  death  rate. 
The  mortality  of  bees  in  the  control  cage  was  19  per  cent,  as  compared  with 
69  per  cent  in  the  cages  containing  the  trees  sprayed  with  lime  sulphur  and 
lead  arsenate,  and  48  per  cent  in  the  cage  containing  the  tree  dusted  with  sulphur 
and  lead  arsenate. 

Doane  (1923)  described  a  number  of  experiments  conducted  at  Stanford 
University.  An  apple  tree  almost  in  full  bloom  was  sprayed  with  arsenate  of 
lead,  3  pounds  of  the  poison  to  50  gallons  of  water,  and  applied  at  the  rate  of 
8  gallons  of  the  spray  per  tree  at  a  pressure  of  from  150  to  200  pounds;  special 
effort  was  made  to  fill  the  calyx  cups  as  far  as  possible.  The  tree  was  then 
tented  and  two  days  later  a  moderately  strong  colony  of  bees  was  placed  beside 
the  tree.  Before  liberating  the  bees  on  the  following  morning,  2  gallons  more 
of  the  spray  were  applied  to  the  tree  in  the  form  of  a  fine  mist  so  as  to  cover 
the  leaves  and  petals.  Shortly  after  being  liberated,  some  of  the  bees  visited 
the  blossoms  and  by  noon  scores  of  them  were  feeding  freely.  Three  days  later 
the  colony  was  returned  to  the  apiary  and,  on  examination,  the  bees  were  found 
to  be  working  normally,  that  honey  had  been  stored  while  working  the  sprayed 
bloom,  that  the  larvae  were  in  a  normal  condition  and  that  the  queen  had  been 
laying  eggs.  During  the  three  days  the  bees  had  been  feeding  on  the  poisoned 
bloom  only  a  few  had  died  which  on  examination  showed  -00000255  grams  of 
arsenic  per  bee.  The  bees  that  were  collected  while  still  living  showed  -000002 
grams  of  arsenic  per  bee.  Bees  from  the  check  tent  contained  -0000006  grams 
of  arsenic  per  bee. 

These  experiments  were  repeated  several  times  with  other  colonies  and  no 
abnormal  effects  noted.  In  a  later  experiment  a  tree  just  coming  into  bloom 
and  in  which  bees  were  feeding  was  sprayed  with  dry  acid  lead  arsenate,  4 
pounds  per  50  gallons.  While  most  of  the  bees  were  driven  away  by  the  spray, 
they  returned  within  ten  minutes  after  spraying  was  stopped.  The  weather 
was  warm,  favourable  for  the  activity  of  the  bees.  No  poisoning  was  observed. 
An  examination  of  the  hive  showed  that  the  bees  and  brood  were  in  good  con- 
dition and  that  the  bees  had  stored  honey  during  the  time  they  fed  on  the  treated 
bloom. 

The  conclusion  arrived  at  is  that  there  is  no  danger  of  poisoning  bees  by 
spraying  fruit  trees  when  they  are  in  bloom,  a  procedure  that  is  common  in 
certain  parts  of  California. 

Merrill  (1924)  criticises  the  methods  employed  by  Doane  and  concludes 
that  the  work  of  Price  is  more  scientific  and  the  results  more  likely  to  be 
correct. 

Tietz  (1924)  has  studied  the  question  of  the  solubility  of  arsenate  of  lead 
in  the  digestive  fluids  of  the  honey  bee.  He  points  out,  that  in  order  that  a 
poison  may  be  absorbed  by  the  body  and  so  cause  death,  it  is  necessary  that 


160 

it  should  be  in  a  soluble  form.  When  arsenate  of  lead  is  taken  into  the  alimen- 
tary tract  of  an  insect,  it  is  very  insoluble.  The  average  arsenate  of  lead  powder 
contains  about  32  per  cent  arsenic  pentoxide,  and  as  the  insect  consumes  but  a 
small  quantity  of  the  spray,  the  solubility  must  increase  when  the  powder  comes 
in  contact  with  the  digestive  fluids  in  the  alimentary  tract;  otherwise  the  quan- 
tity of  arsenic  capable  of  assimilation  would  be  so  small  that  the  insect  would 
be  unharmed  by  its  presence  in  the  blood. 

The  solubility  of  arsenate  of  lead  powder  in  water  was  taken  as  the  unit 
of  solubility.  The  following  conclusions  were  drawn  from  the  experiment:  (1) 
The  solubility  of  arsenate  of  lead  does  not  seem  to  increase  when  the  powder 
is  acted  upon  by  the  fluids  in  the  oesophagus.  (2)  The  digestive  secretions  of 
the  honey  stomach  and  stomach  render  the  poison  at  least  one  and  one-quarter 
times  as  soluble.  (3)  The  action  of  the  intestinal  juices  is  to  throw  at  least 
three  and  three-quarter  times  as  much  of  the  powder  in  solution  as  would  be 
dissolved  by  water  alone. 

Mclndoo  and  Demuth  (1926)  have  conducted  careful  and  extensive  experi- 
ments upon  the  effects  on  honey  bees  of  spraying  fruit  trees  with  arsenicals. 
They  also  give  an  excellent  review  of  work  carried  out  up  to  the  data  of  publica- 
tion. Experiments  at  Winchester,  Va.,  in  1914,  in  which  3  small  isolated  orchards 
in  bloom  were  sprayed  with  (a)  Paris  green,  (b)  lead  arsenate  and  (c)  lead 
arsenate  and  lime  sulphur,  brought  out  the  following  points:  (1)  Bees  work  as 
well  on  sprayed  as  on  unsprayed  bloom;  (2)  they  do  not  fly  away  very  much 
from  the  sprayed  orchard  if  it  is  well  isolated;  (3)  they  are  slightly  affected 
when  a  small  orchard  is  sprayed  in  full  bloom;  and  (4)  the  arsenate  of  lead-lime 
sulphur  mixture  was  found  best  for  experimental  purposes.  Larger  experiments 
conducted  the  same  season  in  a  large  isolated  commercial  orchard  at  Winthrop. 
Me.,  using  the  latter  mixture,  caused  serious  losses  to  the  bees,  pollen  proving  to 
be  the  main  source  of  poisoning.  While  never  more  than  traces  were  found  in  the 
partially  ripened  honey,  chemical  analysis  of  the  nectaries  revealed  traces  of 
arsenic.  None  of  the  ten  colonies  used  was  killed  outright,  but  five  were 
depopulated  in  a  very  short  time  and  five  were  weakened.  The  foregoing  results 
were  obtained  by  daily  observations,  the  use  of  dead-bee  traps  and  analyses 
of  samples.  In  later  experiments  a  method  was  followed  whereby  the  actual 
weight  of  the  bees  could  be  determined  daily.  Experiments  conducted  when 
the  trees  were  sprayed  at  the  ordinary  time,  i.e.,  when  90  per  cent  of  the  blossoms 
had  fallen,  gave  little  or  no  poisoning.  Laboratory  experiments  established  that 
a  fatal  dose  of  arsenic  (As)  per  bee  is  about  0-0004  or  0-0005  milligrams. 

Hilgendorff  and  Borchert  (1926)  describe  observations  in  which  the  applica- 
tion of  arsenical  compounds,  by  aeroplane,  to  forests  for  the  control  of  the  "  fir 
noctuid  "  moth  and  "  nun"  moth  caused  a  serious  bee  mortality  to  the  Sorauer 
beekeepers  and  claims  for  damages  were  instituted.  Chemical  analysis  of  the 
bees  snowed  living  bees  to  contain  -00015  mg.  of  arsenic;  washed  dead  bees. 
•00025  to  -004  mg.;  pollen,  -0001  to  -0005  per  cent,  while  bees,  pollen  and  honey 
from  districts  not  subjected  to  arsenical  preparations  contained  no  arsenic.  The 
quantities  of  arsenic  in  the  pollen  were  quite  sufficient  to  cause  bee  mortality,  as 
shown  later  by  feeding  experiments  with  sugar  solutions  containing  arsenic  in 
proportion  to  that  found  in  the  pollen. 

Bourne  (1927)  presents  considerable  experimental  evidence  with  respect  to 
the  poisoning  of  honey  bees  by  orchard  sprays.  Experiments  were  conducted 
to  ascertain  whether  bees  were  attracted  or  repelled  by  common  spray  mixtures, 
and  whether  the  latter  had  any  injurious  effect  on  the  bees.  First,  a  single  frame 
nucleus  in  an  observation  hive  was  fed  for  a  few  days  on  dilute  honey,  then 
on  a  mixture  of  honey,  one  part,  and  spray  mixture  <  lead  arsenate,  lime  sulphur 
and  nicotine  sulphate)  one  part.  They  readily  accepted  the  honey,  but  were 
repelled  by  the  mixture.     Only  a  few  fed  on  it  and  these,  after  being  isolated. 


161 

died  in  24  hours.  When  only  lead  arsenate  was  used  they  showed  no  reluctance 
in  feeding.  The  colony  was  exterminated  after  feeding  on  the  last  mixture  for 
nine  days.  A  combination  of  lead  arsenate,  lime  sulphur  and  honey  was  also 
accepted  readily;  in  fact,  the  nicotine  sulphate  seemed  to  be  the  only  repellent, 
as  all  combinations  not  containing  it  were  eaten  freely.  All  three  substances 
proved  toxic,  lead  arsenate  the  most  so,  and  the  combination  of  all  three  was  most 
deadly.  A  three-frame  nucleus  with  eggs,  brood  and  some  stores  was  then  placed 
in  a  greenhouse,  and  supplied  with  bloom  sprayed  with  the  regular  spray  com- 
bination. The  bees  were  repelled  by  the  sprayed  bloom,  but  still  worked  it  in 
considerable  numbers.  The  mortality  rose  to  about  390  per  day  within  three 
days  after  exposure  to  sprayed  blossoms,  whereas  it  had  been  from  7  to  63  per 
day,  before.  A  normal  ten-frame  colony  was  then  placed  in  an  orchard  which 
was  sprayed  with  a  combination  of  lead  arsenate,  1\  lbs.  to  50  gallons,  lime  sul- 
phur, 1-40,  and  nicotine  sulphate  1-1000  when  the  centre  blossoms  had  just 
opened.  Bees  which  actually  worked  the  bloom  were  poisoned,  but  owing  to  the 
repellent  action  of  the  spray  and  the  rapid  opening  of  unsprayed  blossoms,  dam- 
age to  the  hive  was  negligible.    An  early  calyx  spray  gave  the  same  result. 

Another  colony  was  placed  in  a  tent,  which  was  erected  over  a  12-year-old 
peach  tree.  After  being  left  for  a  few  days  and  when  considerable  bloom  had 
opened,  the  tree  was  sprayed  with  the  combined  spray;  another  application  was 
made  at  full  bloom.  No  high  mortality  was  noted  except  following  a  spray  of 
lead  arsenate  alone.  Unfavourable  weather  conditions  which  prevented  the  bees 
from  working  may  have  been  partly  responsible. 

Under  conditions  in  Massachusetts,  the  orchard  sprays  applied  nearest  the 
period  of  bloom  are  the  "  pink  "  and  the  "  calyx."  No  sprays  are  scheduled  to 
be  made  when  trees  are  in  full  bloom.  Neither  of  these  sprays,  made  when  there 
was  considerable  bloom  on  the  trees,  caused  any  serious  mortality  of  colonies 
located  in  the  sprayed  orchards.  Following  the  "  late  pink  "  trees  soon  came  into 
full  bloom;  after  the  "  early  calyx,"  the  bees  repelled  by  the  spray  doubtless 
foraged  in  neighbouring  orchards.  In  both  cases  they  found  an  abundance  of 
unpoisoned  bloom  upon  which  to  work.  This  would  indicate  that  improper 
spraying  must  be  carried  out  on  a  large  scale  visibly  to  affect  colonies  not  sub- 
ject to  any  restrictions  of  flight. 

Borchert  (1930)  has  investigated  the  action  of  copper  compounds  commonly 
used,  both  sprays  and  dusts,  also  copper  sulphate  and  basic  copper  carbonate, 
on  bees.  The  poisonous  effect  was  found  to  be  less,  the  more  firmly  the  substance 
clung  to  the  bee's  body.  Poisoning  was  through  the  mouth  in  all  cases;  evidently 
the  bees  swallow  the  substances  in  cleaning  themselves,  and  are  not  so  likely  to 
be  poisoned  if  they  cannot  easily  remove  them.  The  lethal  dose  for  a  bee  was 
found  to  be  equivalent  to  about  0-009  mg.  of  metallic  copper.  (Thus,  dust  or 
spray  substance  containing  one  pound  of  metallic  copper  would  suffice  to  poison 
about  53,000,000  bees.) 

Philp  and  Vansell  (1932)  state  that  spraying  fruit  trees  results  in  con- 
taminated nectar  and  pollen,  as  well  as  cover  crops.  Bees  also  drink  poisoned 
dew  from  leaves,  so  that  even  careful  spraying  cannot  entirely  eliminate  poison- 
ing, particularly  where  cover  crops  exist. 

C.  DEVELOPMENT  OF  PROBLEM 

Previous  to  1916,  we  have  little  evidence  regarding  the  condition  of  the 
bee  industry.  In  that  year  a  total  of  70  apiarists  were  visited  in  the  territory 
to  which  our  studies  have  been  largely  confined,  i.e.,  the  area  bounded  by  Grand 
Pre  on  the  east  and  Berwick  on  the  west.  No  further  survey  was  made  until 
1919  when  only  58  were  visited,  mostly  those  that  had  been  visited  before. 
Thus,  we  have  no  records  previous  to  1916,  and  only  incomplete  records  for 
the  years  1916  and  1919.    We  cannot  now  determine,  therefore,  all  the  apiaries 

60796—11 


162 

that  were  in  existence  at  that  time  nor  the  subsequent  history  of  all  of  these 
apiaries,  but  we  now  have  accurate  knowledge  of  the  number  at  present  exist- 
ing and  can  trace  the  history  of  the  great  majority. 

Of  the  total  number  of  farms  known  to  be  supplied  with  bees  in  1916-1919, 
fifty-nine  had  none  in  1930.  Of  the  remainder,  one  represents  the  Experimental 
Station  apiary,  which  has  only  been  kept  in  existence  by  the  most  assiduous 
care;  at  least  two  are  known  to  practise  migratory  bee-keeping  and  eleven  are  in 
the  towns  of  Wolfville  and  Kentville  where  poisoning,  as  would  be  expected,  is  at 
a  minimum.  One  man  who  formerly  had  twenty  colonies  now  has  three.  With 
these  exceptions  all  others  have  disappeared.  The  apiaries  that  have  been 
wiped  out  include  some  that  were  the  largest  in  the  Valley,  one  having  once 
contained  100  colonies  and  one  40.  The  only  apiarists  who  operate  on  anything 
approaching  a  commercial  scale  have  been  obliged  to  adopt  migratory  bee- 
keeping. The  above  would  indicate  a  mortality  much  greater  than  would  be 
expected  from  normal  causes,  especially  as  the  matter  of  foul  brood  among  the 
bees  has  been  kept  well  in  hand. 

From  time  to  time  new  apiaries  have  sprung  up,  and  it  will  be  interesting 
to  note  what  happened  to  them.  Excluding  the  towns,  the  Experimental  Sta- 
tion and  the  migratory  beekeepers,  we  find  that  16  beekeepers  are  recorded 
for  the  first  time  in  1920.  Fourteen  of  these  apiaries  were  out  of  existence 
in  1930;  one  consisting  of  three  colonies,  was  maintained  at  that  strength;  one 
of  twenty-five  was  reduced  to  eight  and  some  packages  had  been  obtained  by 
purchase.     Figures  for  other  years  might  be  given,  but  they  tell  the  same  story. 

There  is,  no  doubt,  a  large  mortality  among  bees,  due  to  various  factors 
of  which  the  most  important  is  lack  of  efficient  handling.  The  history  of  bee- 
keeping in  the  Valley,  however,  shows  large  losses  among  the  most  experi- 
enced apiarists;  and  apiaries  that  had  been  maintained  successfully  for  many 
years  previous  to  1919,  were  suddenly  and  completely  wiped  out.  In  the  great 
majority  of  cases  the  evidence  points  unmistakably  to  poisoning.  The  only 
beekeepers  retaining  apiaries  of  commercial  size  have  practised  the  removal 
of  their  colonies  throughout  the  danger  period. 

It  would  not  appear  to  be  a  coincidence  that  this  extermination  of  colonies 
took  place  during  and  following  the  year  1919,  since  this  was  the  year  in  which 
the  Valley  fruit-growers  suddenly  adopted  dusting,  a  large  proportion  of  the 
orchards  in  the  Valley  being  dusted  for  the  first  time  in  that  year  and  for  sev- 
eral years  following.  The  figures  of  growers'  purchases  of  such  materials  for  a 
few  typical  years  reveal  this  very  sudden  change  in  practice.  They  also  reveal 
the  increased  quantity  now  being  used  to  cover  the  same  acreage. 

It  should  not  be  assumed,  however,  that  no  trouble  from  poisoning  existed 
before  that  period.  While  it  is  true  that  we  have  few  definite  records  on  this 
point,  it  is  also  true  that  many  beekeepers  regarded  poisoning  from  sprays 
applied  in  bloom  as  one  of  their  greatest  sources  of  trouble.  Nevertheless,  many 
of  them  who  were  quickly  wiped  out  following  the  widespread  adoption  of  the 
dusting  method,  had  been  able  to  maintain  successful  apiaries  for  many  years 
in  spite  of  occasional  poisoning. 

The  increased  amount  of  poisoning  from  the  dusting  method  has  been  gen- 
erally attributed  to  three  causes:  — 

1.  The  greater  drift  of  the  dust  to  surrounding  vegetation,  which  thus  con- 
taminates other  bloom  such  as  dandelion,  wild  radish,  etc.,  growing  in  or  near 
the  orchard. 

2.  The  greater  amount  of  arsenic  applied  per  acre  when  following  this 
method. 

3.  The  difference  in  the  character  of  the  deposit.  Spray  quickly  dries  on 
and  is  difficult  to  remove.  Dust  makes  a  loose  layer  over  leave-  and  blossoms, 
is  gathered  by  or  becomes  attached  to  the  hairs  of  the  bee  and  is  stored  with 
the  pollen. 


163 

With  the  development  of  more  efficient  spray  machines  and  nozzles  deliver- 
ing a  greater  quantity  of  spray  per  minute  and  the  recognition  of  the  fact  that 
dusting  was  giving  inferior  results  against  certain  pests,  there  has  come  a 
decided  swing  back  to  the  spraying  method  during  the  past  four  years.  As  far 
as  can  be  determined  there  has  been  a  sale  of  approximately  90  spraying  out- 
fits per  year  over  this  period  as  compared  with  approximately  three  dusters 
and  the  latter  have  been  used  largely  to  supplement  spraying  practices  in  the 
way  of  additional  fungicide  applications  or  for  the  application  of  nicotine  dust. 
The  new  types  of  spraying  nozzles,  however,  deliver  quite  a  different  type  of 
spray  from  that  of  the  old  type,  being  more  in  the  form  of  a  fog,  which  drifts 
on  to  surrounding  vegetation  to  a  greater  extent  than  could  occur  where  old 
style  nozzles  were  used.  Furthermore,  the  greater  number  of  gallons  used  and 
the  increased  strength  employed  by  many  growers  are  added  factors.  It  is 
not  surprising,  therefore,  that  indications  of  greater  poisoning  of  bees,  clue  to 
spray  applications  have  become  apparent  during  the  past  few  years. 

The  foregoing  is  only  intended  as  a  general  outline  of  the  situation,  in  order 
to  serve  as  a  background  for  the  more  definite  observations  and  experiments  car- 
ried out  since  1928. 

D.  EXPERIMENTS  IN  BEE  POISONING 

1.    FACTORS    INVOLVED    IN    CARRYING    OUT    AND    INTERPRETING 
RESULTS  OF  POISON   STUDIES 

There  are  a  number  of  difficulties  inherent  in  both  tent  and  field  experiments 
with  bees  that  render  it  difficult  properly  to  plan  such  experiments,  and  still 
more  to  interpret  their  results. 

It  is  a  comparatively  easy  matter  to  feed  bees  upon  definitely  measured 
doses  of  various  materials  in  the  laboratory,  but  to  secure  similar  results  under 
tents  or  in  the  open  orchard  is  quite  a  different  problem.  Some  of  the  most 
important  difficulties  are  here  set  forth  in  order  clearly  to  portray  the  limitations 
of  such  work. 

(a)  In  experiments  carried  out  under  tents  covering  trees,  certain  unnatural 
conditions  develop.  Immediately  bees  are  released,  a  large  number  fly  to  the 
top  of  the  tent  and  remain  there  in  spite  of  all  attempts  to  dislodge  them.  There, 
a  number  may  become  chilled  or  die  of  starvation  and  keep  dropping  down  on 
the  floor  of  the  tent,  interfering  with  the  counts  of  dead  bees.  This  trouble  is 
most  apparent  when  periods  of  dull,  cool  weather  prevail.  Clustering  at  the 
top  may  be  partially  prevented  by  covering  the  tops  with  a  dark  sheet. 

Since  the  bees  are  forced  to  feed  upon  the  treated  bloom,  the  results  obtained 
are  liable  to  be  accentuated  over  those  that  would  occur  under  field  conditions. 
At  any  rate,  they  are  not  strictly  comparable,  though,  if  correctly  interpreted 
in  the  light  of  all  relevant  factors,  they  may  yield  results  of  value.  Another 
disturbing  factor  is  that  during  periods  of  rainy  weather,  the  bees  will  lap  up 
the  water  that  drips  from  the  limbs  or  trunk  and  thus  consume  considerable 
quantities  of  the  material  supplied,  dissolved  or  suspended  in  the  water,  which 
they  might  not  do  under  out-of-door  conditions.  This  results  in  materials  not 
ordinarily  considered  violently  poisonous,  sometimes  causing  a  high  mortality. 
Residue  from  previous  sprays,  may,  under  such  conditions,  cause  certain  losses. 

(6)  In  field  experiments  it  is  equally  difficult  to  control  the  conditions  of 
the  test.  Several  workers  speak  of  their  experiments  being  conducted  in 
"  isolated  "  orchards.  We  have  been  quite  unable  to  obtain  isolated  orchards 
for  our  work,  since  in  every  case,  there  are  other  orchards  sufficiently  near  for 
bees  to  visit,  and  one  could  never  be  certain  that  the  results  noted  were  caused 
by  materials  applied  in  the  orchard  in  which  the  bees  were  placed  or  obtained 
from  a  distance. 

60796— 11 J 


164 

The  habit  of  bees  gathering  supplies  from  a  limited  area  has  been  clearly 
brought  out  in  these  experiments.  An  example  might  be  cited  of  a  case  observed 
at  the  Experimental  Station  in  1929.  Only  one  colony  of  the  fifty  showed 
distinct  signs  of  poisoning  and  in  this  case  the  loss  was  heavy.  The  orchard 
was  sprayed  while  there  was  a  certain  amount  of  bloom,  mostly  with  lime 
sulphur-calcium  arsenate.  However,  there  were  a  few  small  plots  devoted  to 
spraying  and  dusting  experiments,  one  of  which  had  received  90-10  sulphur-lead 
arsenate  dust.  Evidently  this  colony  obtained  its  supplies  from  a  location  differ- 
ent from  the  other  bees  and  it  may  have  foraged  chiefly  on  this  block.  In 
several  cases  where  numbers  of  hives  were  standing  together  certain  individual 
colonies  showed  much  worse  poisoning  than  others,  irrespective  of  colony  strength 
or  other  factors. 

(c)  In  both  field  and  tent  experiments  the  condition  of  the  colony  is 
important.  It  is  impossible  to  secure  colonies  of  identical  strength  and  vigour. 
Some  were  noticeably  "  sluggish ",  worked  the  bloom  less  actively,  and  so 
received  less  poison.  The  results  of  varying  weather  conditions  profoundly 
modify  the  results  of  both  tent  and  field  experiments.  These  are  discussed  at 
greater  length  in  another  section.  It  is  sufficient  to  point  out  that  very  erron- 
eous results  are  obtained  if  this  factor  is  ignored. 

2.   POISON    USED,    SYMPTOMS   AND    LETHAL    DOSAGES 

(a)  Of  the  spray  materials  used  in  Nova  Scotia  the  following  are  the  most 
important:  — 

(i)   Food  Poisons: 

Calcium  arsenate 

Arsenate  of  lead 
(ii)   Contact  Poisons: 

Nicotine  sulphate   (liquid) 

Nicotine  sulphate-lime   (dust) 
(iii)   Fungicides: 

Bordeaux  mixture 

Lime  sulphur 

Sulphur  dust 

Copper  lime  dust   ("Bordeaux  dust"). 

Various  combinations  of  these  may  be  used,  the  fungicides  generally  having 
the  arsenical  added,  and  frequently  representatives  of  all  these  groups  are  mixed 
together.  Experiments  under  tents  and  in  the  field  with  these  and  all  com- 
binations used  commercially  were  carried  out. 

(b)  SYMPTOMS   AND   EFFECTS   OF   POISONS 

(i)  Arsenical  Poisons. — The  results  of  arsenical  poisoning  in  whatever  form, 
are  most  apparent.  "  Crawlers  "  soon  appear  in  front  of  the  hive,  their  limbs 
appearing  to  be  partially  paralyzed.  Clumps  of  bees  often  cluster  together  in 
bunches.  The  abdomen  becomes  distended  and  when  subjected  to  pressure  read- 
ily bursts.  A  pronounced  dysentery  is  often  apparent,  the  hives  becoming  badly 
spotted.  This  effect  is  most  pronounced  when  the  arsenic  is  contained  in  a 
sulphur  preparation.  After  a  period  of  crawling,  twisting  and  squirming,  the 
poisoned  bees  gradually  become  immobile  and  only  feebly  respond  to  stimulation 
for  some  time  before  death  ensues. 

It  was  a  notable  fact  that  none  of  the  "  crawlers  "  taken  in  front  of  poisoned 
hives  had  pollen  in  their  pollen  baskets.  They  evidently  entered  the  hive  and 
came  out  to  die  after  having  gotten  rid  of  their  load. 


165 

(ii)  Nicotine. — Certain  cases  of  nicotine  poisoning  under  field  conditions 
are  not  available.  As  nicotine  quickly  volatilizes  and,  when  applied  as  dust, 
is  usually  used  at  night  or  early  morning  when  no  bees  are  flying,  heavy  mortal- 
ity from  this  source  would  not  be  expected.  In  tents,  in  which  heavy  dust 
applications  were  made,  bees  sometimes  became  completely  covered  with  the 
nicotine  lime  dusts  and  some  poisoning  developed.  In  some  years  trees  sprayed 
with  this  material  also  produced  killing.  Definite  feeding  experiments  performed 
by  other  workers  are  available.  It  would  appear  that  symptoms  caused  by  this 
poison  are  not  as  violent  as  those  produced  by  arsenical  sprays.  There  is 
little  motion,  and,  where  the  poisoning  is  insufficient  to  cause  death,  quick 
recovery  results.     Otherwise  complete  paralysis  is  followed  by  death. 

(iii)  Sulphur. — Sulphur  applied  in  the  form  of  sulphide  sprays  or  elemental 
sulphur  in  the  form  of  dust  causes  pronounced  dysentery.  In  tent  experiments  a 
certain  amount  of  mortality  was  caused,  but  under  field  conditions  it  was 
believed  that  most  of  the  bees  recovered.  Severe  spotting  of  clothing  hung  out 
to  dry  has  resulted  from  the  use  of  this  material  in  several  cases. 

(iv)  Copper. — Copper  fungicides  as  yet  have  not  been  definitely  proved  to 
cause  death  to  bees  under  field  conditions  and  the  evidence  would  appear  to 
indicate  that  they  are  quite  safe  to  use.  Where  fed  to  bees  or  when  wet  weather 
prevails  they  may  cause  death.  The  insects  become  sluggish,  lose  their  power  of 
movement  and  finally  death  ensues,  only  more  slowly  than  with  arsenical 
poisons. 

Repellent  Action  of  Sprays. — Lime  sulphur  was  the  only  material  found 
definitely  repellent  by  Mclndoo  and  Demuth  (1926).  Bourne  (1927)  found 
nicotine  also  to  be  repellent.  Repulsion  to  these  materials  is  hard  to  demon- 
strate under  field  conditions  and,  if  it  exists,  it  would  appear  to  have 
little  practical  effect.  In  making  our  observations,  we  have  repeatedly  observed 
the  action  of  different  materials  including  poisoned  Bordeaux,  lime  sulphur 
alone  or  in  combinations  with  calcium  arsenate,  or  with  the  latter  and  nicotine 
sulphate,  90-10  sulphur-lead  arsenate  and  other  combinations.  We  have  also 
studied  the  effect  of  the  commercial  materials  and  combinations  when  applied 
to  bouquets  and,  in  all  cases,  could  obtain  little  consistent  evidence  of  pro- 
longed repulsion  under  field  conditions,  the  bees  sometimes  settling  quickly  upon 
the  trees  as  fast  as  the  machine  passed.  In  few  cases  was  the  repulsion  of  long 
duration.  In  one  case  lime  sulphur-calcium  arsenate  appeared  to  repel  for  17 
hours.  In  most  cases  the  repulsion  was  of  only  a  few  minutes  duration  and 
was  apparently  over  by  the  time  the  material  had  dried.  Sulphur-lead  arsenate 
actually  seemed  to  attract.  These  observations  are  based  on  definite  counts  of 
wTild  and  hive  bees  visiting  bloom  treated  with  different  materials,  unsprayed 
bloom  being  available  in  all  cases. 

(c)  LETHAL   DOSAGES 

Data  obtained  from  analyses  of  bees  (package  and  hive)  that  had  died 
from  natural  causes  and  those  dying  in  check  tents,  where  no  poison  had  been 
applied,  showed  arsenic  present,  as  metallic  arsenic,  from  -00004  to  -00008 
milligrams  of  internal  arsenic  (As),  and  from  -00004  to  -00009  milligrams  of 
total  arsenic  (As),  internal  plus  external,  per  bee.  No  traces  of  arsenic  were 
found  in  check  samples  of  larvae,  pollen  or  nectar,  but  minute  quantities  were 
found  in  pupae. 

From  this  data  we  may  assume  that  when  the  internal  arsenic  found  in 
adult  bees  is  greater  than  -00004  milligrams  of  metallic  arsenic  per  bee,  poison- 
ing may  be  suspected;  definitely  so,  when  higher  than  -00008  milligrams  of 
metallic  arsenic  per  bee  are  detected.     Analyses  of  adult  bees,  larvae,  pollen, 


166 

pupae  and  nectar  from  locations  where  no  poison  of  any  kind  was  used  and 
where  no  poisoning  was  obtained,  are  given  for  purposes  of  comparison  with 
subsequent  observations   in  which   poisoning   actually   occurred. 


TABLE  No.  28.— ANALYSES  OF  CHECK  SAMPLES  OF  BEES,  LARVAE,  POLLEN,  PUPAE 

AND  NECTAR 


Sample 

No. 

Nature 

of 
material 

From 

bees 

(Internal) 

Mg. 

Wash 
water  from 

bees 
(External) 

Mg. 

Total 
internal 

plus 
external 

Mg. 

Average 
arsenic 

(As) 

per  bee 

(Internal) 

Mg. 

Remarks 

1 

Bees 

•003030 
(bees  not 
washed) 

•002500 
(bees  not 
washed) 

•004050 
(bees  not 
washed) 

•001273 

•004303 
•001270 

•003500 
•004550 

•001750 
•004022 

•00003 
•00006 

•00003 
•00005 

•00004 
•00004 

nil. 
•00007 
nil. 
nil. 
nil. 
nil. 
nil. 

100  bees  used  in    each  sample 
except  No.  la,  when  19  bees 
were  taken. 

la 

u 

2 

u 

•001000 

2a 

« 

3 
3a 

•000700 

4 

Larvae 

5 

Pupae 

•00008  %;  10  pupae  used. 
1-5390  gs.  used. 

6 

Pollen 

7 

0-9038  g.  used. 

8 

Nectar 

Nectar  pipetted  from  combs; 

9 

approximately  3    grams    in 
each  sample. 

10 

tt 

3.  TENT  EXPERIMENTS  IN   BEE  POISONING 

In  1928-1929  tents  were  placed  over  entire  trees,  which  were  sprayed 
during  bloom,  and  a  hive  of  bees  placed  in  the  tent  immediately  thereafter. 
In  1930  and  1931  large  bouquets  were  used  instead  of  entire  trees.  In  view  of 
the  great  amount  of  data  secured  and  the  limitations  in  accuracy  from  such 
experiments,  only  the  more  outstanding  results  are  included  in  the  following 
report. 

Following  the  application,  observations  as  to  the  reaction  of  the  bees  to  the 
different  materials  and  on  the  daily  mortality  were  made.  The  dead  bees  were 
counted  every  day  from  the  floor  of  the  tent  upon  which  sheets  were  placed, 
until  mortality  ceased.  Following  1930,  "  dead-bee  traps  "  of  the  type  used 
by  Mclndoo  (1926)  were  employed,  but  modified  so  as  to  have  a  removable 
bottom  board  from  which  the  top  could  be  lifted  and  the  dead  bees  readily 
removed.  After  pronounced  poisoning  had  developed,  we  removed  the  bees 
from  the  tents,  placed  the  traps  in  place  and  based  further  counts  on  daily 
removals  of  dead  bees  from  the  traps. 

Chemical  tests  were  made  of  dead  bees  and  of  brood  available.  Sufficient 
pollen  or  nectar  was  rarely  available  from  bees  tented  over  a  single  tree;  but 
pollen  and  nectar  from  bad  cases  of  field  poisoning  are  reported  on  elsewhere. 


(a)  TECHNIQUE  OF  CHEMICAL  TESTS 

When  the  dead  bees  were  received  at  the  laboratory  two  chemical  pro- 
cedures were  followed.  In  one  instance,  the  bees  were  analyzed  as  received — 
no  attempt  being  made  to  remove  external  poison,  the  arsenic  present  in  adhering 
dust  or  other  forms  on  the  body  of  the  bee. 

In  the  procedure  generally  followed,  the  bees,  usually  100  in  number,  were 
placed  in  a  suitable  bottle  and  washed,  by  gentle  agitation,  with  4-25  ml.  aliquota 


167 

of  2  per  cent  nitric  acid  (by  volume).  After  each  washing  the  liquid  or  wash 
water  was  poured  through  a  filter.  The  bees  were  then  washed  in  the  filter  with 
20-30  ml.  of  distilled  water,  and  finally  with  10  ml.  of  alcohol. 

For  destruction  of  organic  matter,  the  samples  of  pollen,  nectar,  larvae, 
pupae,  bees  as  received,  wash  water  and  washed  bees  with  filter  paper  were 
treated  with  10-15  ml.  of  cone,  sulphuric  acid  and  charred  on  the  hot  plate. 
Concentrated  nitric   acid   was   then   added,   the   liquid   heated,    and   more   acid 


.  r 


Fig.  69. —  (1)    Modified  Mclndoo  trap,  showing  removable   bottom  board;    (2) 
modified  Mclndoo  trap  in  place  (original  from  photo  by  C.  B.  Gooderham) . 


added  until  finally  a  colourless  solution  was  obtained.  This  solution  was  evapor- 
ated to  dryness,  washed  into  a  modified  Gutzeit  generating  flask,  and  arsenic 
determined  by  the  Gutzeit  method. 

The  arsenic  found  on  analysis  of  the  washed  bees  is  designated  internal 
arsenic;  that  from  analysis  of  the  wash  water,  external  arsenic — the  sum  of  the 
two,  total  arsenic.  The  total  arsenic  is  also  obtained  by  analyzing  the  bees 
direct  as  received,  e.g.,  not  washing  with  nitric  acid  previous  to  analysis. 


168 
(b)  EXPERIMENTS  IN  1928 

The  following  materials  and  combinations  were  utilized  in  1928  and  the 
results  of  the  tests  from  a  standpoint  of  mortality  and  chemical  analysis  are 
found  in  the  accompanying  tables.  Only  those  materials  that  gave  a  greater 
mortality  than  the  checks  are  shown  in  the  table.  All  others  gave  either  the 
same  results   or   a   lesser   mortality. 

The  following  spray  dust  preparations  were  applied  to  the  tented  trees:  — 


Tent  No. 

i 

No. 

' 

No. 

1 

No. 

( 

No. 

t 

No. 

t 

No. 

' 

No. 

1 

No. 

t 

No. 

Sprays 
1— Bordeaux  3-10-40. 
2— Nicotine  sulphate   (1  pt.-lOO  gals.) 
3 — Calcium  arsenate   (1-40). 

4— Bordeaux  (3-10-40) -Nicotine  sulphate  (1  pt.-lOO  gals.) 
5— Bordeaux  (3-10-40) -Calcium    arsenate    (1-40). 

6 — Bordeaux  (3-10-40) -Nicotine  sulphate  (1  pt.-lOO  gals.) -Calcium  arsenate  (1-40) 
7 — Lime  sulphur  (1-40). 

8 — Lime  sulphur  (1-40) -Nicotine   sulphate    (1   pt.-lOO   gals.) 
9 — Lime  sulphur  (l-40)-Calcium  arsenate  (1-40) 
10 — Lime  sulphur  (1-40-Calcium  arsenate   (1-40)-Nicotine  sulphate  (1  pt.-lOO  gals.) 


Dusts 
Tent  No.  11— Sulphur    (95-5). 

"     No.  12 — Nicotine-(5f/t    nicotine  sulphate). 

"      No.  13— Sulphur-Lead  arsenate    (90-10). 

"     No.  14— Poisoned  Bordeaux   (12-8-80). 

"     No.  15 — Copper  carbonate-Lime-Nicotine  sulphate    (9-86-5). 

"      No.  16— Check. 

"      No.  17— Check. 

(i)  Results  from  Standpoint  of  Mortality,  1928. — The  most  outstanding 
point  in  connection  with  these  results  was  that  all  the  combinations  containing 
nicotine  showed  lower  mortality  than  the  checks,  except  Xo.  10,  which  included 
calcium  arsenate.  All  Bordeaux  combinations  gave  less  than  the  checks,  except 
No.  5,  which  contained  calcium  arsenate.  All  mixtures  containing  lime  sulphur, 
except  No.  8,  which  contained  nicotine  sulphate,  and  sulphur  dust  gave  a  higher 
mortality  than  the  checks,  even  plain  lime  sulphur  showing  some  loss.  Sulphur 
dust  caused  dysentery,  but  less  mortality  than  where  arsenicals  were  present. 
Sulphur-lead  arsenate  was  particularly  deadly  and  sudden  in  its  action,  a  fact 
which  checks  up  with  field  data  and  chemical  tests.  Only  arsenical  and  arsenical 
combinations  produced  dead  brood. 


TABLE  No.  29. 


-TABLE  SHOWING  TOTAL  MORTALITY  OX  2nd,  3rd.  4th 
OF  COUNT 


AND  FINAL  DAY 


No. 


Material 


Total  number  Dead  on  Different  Days 


2nd. 


3rd. 


4th. 


1-  ina  1 
Total 


1  13 

2  10 

3  3 

4  9 

5  5 

6  11 

7  7 

8  17 


Sulphur-lead  arsenate  dust 

Lime  sulphur-Calcium  arsenate- Nicotine  sulphate 

Calcium  arsenate 

Lime  sulphur-(  'aleium  arsenate 

Bordeaux-Calcium  arsenate 

Sulphur  dust 

Lime  sulphur 

Check 


200 
0 
0 
0 
0 

242 

0 

52 


1,900 
380 
698 
405 
304 
342 
349 
42 


,900 
663 

.512 
819 
516 
384 
504 


4,083 

3,368 
3,284 
2,069 

1.9S1 
1.773 
1,483 
1,230 


(ii)  Chemical  Data,  1928.— Taking  -00004  to  -00008  milligrams  oi  metallic 
arsenic  as  the  lethal  dose  per  bee,  we  find  a  condition  of  bee  poisoning  to  exist 
on  May  31  and  June  1,  the  first  days'  mortality,  as  follows:  — 


169 


Tent  No.    3 — Calcium  arsenate- •  00086  milligrams  per  bee. 

"  No.    5 — Bordeaux-Calcium  arsenate- -00099  milligrams  per  bee. 

"  No.    6 — Bordeaux-Calcium  arsenate-Nicotine  sulphate-- 00046  milligrams  per  bee. 

"  No.     9 — Lime  sulphur-Calcium  arsenate-. 00036  milligrams  per  bee. 

"  No.  10 — Lime  sulphur-Calcium  arsenate-Nicotine  sulphate- -00045  milligrams  per  bee. 

"  No.  13 — Sulphur-Lead  arsenate- -00106  milligrams  per  bee. 

"  No.  14— Bordeaux  (12-8-80)- -00042  milligrams  per  bee. 

It  will  be  seen  that  in  all  cases  the  minimum  lethal  dose  is  greatly  exceeded 
in  these  samples.  While  the  bees  collected  from  the  check  tents  in  this  experi- 
ment showed,  from  -00014  to  -00017  milligrams  of  metallic  arsenic  per  bee,  this 
high  figure  is  due  to  poison  residue  obtained  from  previous  sprays  and  apparently 
imbibed  in  drip  water  consumed  by  bees  following  rains.  Later  work  has  shown 
that  from  -00004  to  '00008  milligrams  of  metallic  arsenic  represents  more 
closely  the  quantity  of  arsenic  per  bee  that  dies  from  natural  causes.  Subse- 
quent analyses  of  adult  bees,  together  with  larva?  and  pollen  from  certain  of  the 
tests  showing  heaviest  mortality,  were  made,  and  the  data  presented  in  the 
following  table.  Owing  to  the  comparatively  small  amount  of  forage  afforded 
by  a  single  tree,  it  was  only  possible  to  secure  larvae  and  pollen  samples  from  a 
few  tents  and  no  nectar  samples  were  procurable. 


TABLE  No.   30.- 


-ARSENIC,  AS   METALLIC   As,    FROM   CHEMICAL   ANALYSES   OF  BEES 
UNDER  CAGE  CONDITIONS,  1928 


Number 

of  bees 

analysed 

Arsenic  (As) 

in  milligrams 

Tent  number  and  date  of  collection 

From  bees 

(a) 

Wash  water 
from  bees 

(b) 

Total 

bees  plus 

wash  water 

(c) 

Average 
quantity  of 
arsenic  (As) 

per  bee 
(d) 

17  Check  May  31 

40 
100 
100 
100 
100 
100 
100 
100 
100 
100 
100 
100 
100 
100 
100 
100 
100 

50 
100 
100 
100 

•00682 
•01439 
•08633 
•11510 
•04392 
•10602 
•12722 
•09996 
•04241 
•10299 
•04544 
•03636 
•10602 
•04241 
•04544 
•21507 
•10602 
•17268 
•04847 
•15752 
•08197 

•00341 

(e) 
•01212 

(e) 
•05755 

(f) 

(f) 
•04847 

(f) 

(f) 

(f) 
•04241 

(e) 

(f) 

•09694 
•06816 
•08028 
•03938 
•05756 

(e) 

(f) 

•01023 
•01439 
•09845 
•11510 
•10147 

•00017 

16        "       June  1 

3       "       May  31 

•00014 
•00086 

3       "       May  31 

3       "       June  1 

•00044 

•00106 

3       "       June  7 

•00127 

5       "       May  31 

•14843 

•  00099 

•  00042 

6       "       June  4 

•00103 

6       "       June  6. . . 

■00045 

9       "       May  31 

•07877 
•10602 

•00036 

9       "       June  1 

9       "       June  4.. 

•00042 

10       "       June  1 

•14238 
•28323 
•18630 
•21206 
•10603 
•15752 

•00045 

13       "       May  30 

•00215 

13       "       May  31 

•00106 

13       "       May  31 

■00345 

13       "       June  4 

■00048 

13       "       June  4. . . 

•00082 

(a)  Total  quantity  of  arsenic  (As)  within  the  body  of  the  bee. 

(b)  Arsenic  (As)  carried  outside  the  body  of  the  bee. 

(c)  Includes  arsenic  (As)  within  and  without  the  body  of  the  bee. 

(d)  Average  quantity  of  arsenic  (As)  per  bee  within  the  body. 

(e)  Bees  not  washed  previous  to  examination.     (Internal  and  external  (As)). 

(f)  Washed  bees  only  examined.     (Arsenic  (As)  within  the  body  of  the  bee). 

TABLE  No.  31.— ARSENIC,  AS  METALLIC  As,  FROM  LARVAE,  1928 


Source  of  Poisoning 

Number 

Weight 

Arsenic  (As) 

Per  larva 

No.  3  Calcium  arsenate — June  8. . . . 

4 
15 
30 

gm. 
0131 
0-707 
2-633 

p.c. 
•00173 
•00114 
•00150 

mg. 
•  000568 

No.  9  Lime  sulphur-calcium  arsenate — June  6 

•000529 

No.  13  Sulphur-lead  arsenate — June  5 

•001313 

170 
(c)  EXPERIMENTS    IN    1929 

Tent  studies  were  continued  in  1929  along  similar  lines  to  those  of  the 
preceding  year.  It  was  found  impossible  to  start  all  the  experiments  at  once; 
consequently  it  was  necessary  to  initiate  a  series  on  two  distinct  dates. 
Weather  conditions  are  believed  to  have  played  a  greater  part  in  some  cases 
in  the  results  secured  than  the  actual  differences  between  materials.  Analyses 
of  bees  from  all  the  tents  were  made,  as  in  1928.  In  order  to  avoid  repetition 
and  conserve  space,  however,  the  principal  points  brought  out  in  the  two  years' 
tests  are  shown  in  the  accompanying  table,  and  analyses  of  pupae  from  certain 
of  the  poisoned  colonies,  in  the  table  which  follows  it.  Analyses  of  pollen  col- 
lected during  this  season  are  shown  elsewhere. 

TABLE  No.  32.— SUMMARY  OF  RESULTS  WITH  ADULT  BEES 


No. 

Material 

Order  of 
mortality1 

Average  arsenic  per  bee, 
milligrams 

1928 

1929 

1928 

1929 

1 

B 

X 
X 

3 
X 

5 
X 

7 
X 

4 

2 

6 
X 

1 
X 
X 
X 

X 
X 

4 
X 

2 
X 

3 
X 

7 
6 
5 
X 

X 
X 
X 

X 
X 

•00044-00127 

X 
•00042-00099 
•00042-00103 

X 

X 
•00036-00042 
•00045 

X 

X 
•00048-00215 

X 

X 

X 

X 

2 

N.S 

X 

3 

C.A.... 

•00003- -00050 

4 

B.  +  N.S 

X 

5 

B.  +  C.A 

•00004- -00023 

6 

B.  +  C.A.  +  N.S 

•00008- -00018 

7 

L.S 

X 

8 

L.S.  +  N.S 

X 

9 

L.S.  +  C.A 

•00003- -00018 

10 

L.S.  +  C.A.  +  N.S 

•00006- -00018 

11 

S.  Dust 

X 

12 

N.S.  Dust  (2%) 

X 

13 

S.  +  L.A.  Dust 

•00011-00055 

14 

B.  +  C.A.  Dust 

X 

15 

CuC.  +  N.S.  Dust 

X 

16 

Check.... 

•00004-00008 

B.     =  Bordeaux;  N.S. 

C.A.     =  Calcium  arsenate;  L.S. 

L.A.      =  Lead  arsenate;  CuC. 

S.  -  Sulphur. 
irThe  material  showing  highest  mortality  rate  is  numbered. 
X.  Less  mortality  than  checks. 


=   Nicotine  sulphate; 

=  Lime  sulphur; 

=  Copper  carbonate; 


TABLE  No.  33.— ARSENIC  AS  METALLIC  As,  FROM  PUPAE,  1929 


Num- 
ber 

Weight 

Arsenic  (As) 

Source  of  Poisoning 

Milli- 
grams 

No.    3  Calcium  arsenate.  .  . 

3 
13 

gms. 
•0932 
•9060 

p.c. 
•00244 
•00025 

per  pupa 
•00076 

No.  16  Check 

June  1 1 

•00018 

(d)  SUMMARY  AND  CONCLUSIONS  FROM  TENT  STUDIES  OF  BEE  POISONING. 

1928-1929 

The  inconsistent  mortality  from  the  different  spray  materials  in  the  different 
years  indicates  that  we  cannot  draw  definite  deductions  from  one  season's  opera- 
tions, since  difference  between  colonies,  effects  of  weather  conditions  and  other 
factors  lead  to  highly  irregular  results.  With  the  dust  preparations,  Bordeaux 
dust  causes  some  mortality,  sulphur  dust  heavy  mortality  where  the  bees  are 
confined,  probably  due  to  the  severe  dysentery  produced,  but  the  highest  mortal- 


171 

ity  is  caused  by  the  use  of  sulphur-lead  arsenate  dust.  The  high  mortality  from 
this  combination  whether  90-10  or  85-15  has  been  unquestionably  demonstrated 
year  after  year. 

Owing  to  the  situation  outlined,  conclusions  on  all  points  can  be  attempted 
only  with  caution;  but,  making  allowance  for  all  irregularities  and  inconsistencies, 
and  bearing  in  mind  the  field  evidence  available,  the  following  general  conclusions 
appear  justifiable: 

1.  Sprays  containing  arsenicals  are  dangerous  to  bees  when  applied  during 
bloom. 

2.  Only  sprays  or  dusts  containing  arsenicals  caused  dead  brood  in  these 
tests. 

3.  The  combination  causing  the  most  sudden  and  the  heaviest  mortality 
was  sulphur-lead  arsenate  in  dust  form. 

4.  Generally,  but  not  invariably,  less  toxicity  appears  to  be  exhibited  by 
sprays  or  dusts  containing  nicotine  or  copper  sulphate  as  an  ingredient. 

5.  Unpoisoned  sulphur  dust,  in  addition  to  causing  a  pronounced  dysentery, 
may  also  cause  the  death  of  bees.  Just  how  important  this  may  be  under 
orchard  conditions  is  not  clear,  but  apparently  it  is  much  less  dangerous  and 
slower  in  action  than  arsenicals. 

(e)  REPULSION  TESTS  1930-1931 

Tent  studies  were  continued  in  1930-1931  mainly  with  the  object  of  securing 
information  regarding  the  repellent  action  of  different  spray  and  dust  materials. 
Tests  were  run  in  duplicate,  and  both  treated  and  untreated  bloom  were  avail- 
able to  the  bees ;  tubs  filled  with  limbs  of  bloom  in  good  condition  being  used. 

Counts  were  made  at  intervals  on  the  different  treated  bouquets  to  determine 
the  number  of  hive  and  wild  bees  visiting  each,  as  compared  with  those  visiting 
the  untreated  bouquet  in  the  same  tent.  As  in  the  poison  tests,  the  evidence 
was  to  some  extent  conflicting  in  the  different  years;  and  furthermore,  different 
colonies  reacted  differently  to  the  ingredients  used.  Weather  conditions  again 
had  an  important  influence. 

Careful  study  of  the  data  secured,  indicates  a  few  points  that  appear  to 
be  reasonably  consistent.  Repulsion  from  the  regular  lime  sulphur-arsenate  is 
likely  to  be  very  temporary  and  appears  to  have  little  significance  under  field 
conditions.  The  addition  of  nicotine  seems  to  increase  the  repulsion.  Sulphur- 
lead  arsenate  has  no  appreciable  repulsive  action.  In  fact,  some  observations 
make  it  appear  rather  attractive  than  otherwise.  Bordeaux,  whether  in  dust  or 
in  liquid  form,  appeared  to  exert  marked  repellent  action  in  most  cases  when 
bouquets  were  used;  but  this  was  not  so  evident  under  orchard  conditions. 
Nicotine  dust  also  seemed  to  drive  the  bees  away  and  to  remain  repellent  for 
some  time  after  application.  It  may  be  noted  that  this  dust  is  usually  applied 
at  a  time  of  day  when  bees  are  inactive. 

(/)  FEEDING   TESTS,    1932 

In  order  to  supplement  the  foregoing  studies,  various  spray  ingredients  were 
fed  directly  to  two-frame  nuclei,  the  poison  being  incorporated  in  a  sugar  syrup 
(1-1),  placed  in  honey  can  feeders  and  placed  over  the  frames  in  the  ordinary 
manner.  Dead-bee  traps  were  placed  on  all  the  hives.  The  following  materials 
were  used  at  the  rate  indicated: — 

1.  Calcium  arsenate,  1  pound  to  40  gallons. 

2.  Copper  sulphate,  3  pounds  to  40  gallons. 

3.  Sulphur  dust,  25  pounds  to  100  gallons. 

4.  Nicotine  sulphate,  1  pint  to  100  gallons. 

5.  Lime  sulphur,  1  gallon  to  40  gallons. 


172 

To  summarize  briefly  the  result  of  these  tests,  the  bees  refused  to  feed  upon 
the  syrup  contained  in  Nos.  2,  4  and  o  to  any  appreciable  extent  and  showed 
every  evidence  of  repulsion.  After  the  nicotine  sulphate  had  been  exposed  on 
the  frames  for  some  time,  there  was  a  limited  amount  of  feeding;  but,  as  only  a 
very  few  dead  bees  were  found,  it  was  evident  that  no  extensive  feeding  took 
place.  There  was  no  sign  of  dead  brood.  Though  few  dead  bees  were  found 
in  the  traps,  a  few  larvae  were  found  in  the  combs  two  days  later,  showing  that 
a  slight  amount  of  the  poison  must  have  been  fed  to  the  larvae.  The  gas 
evolved  from  the  lime  sulphur  solution  made  the  bees  restless  at  first,  but  caused 
little,  if  any,  mortality  for  several  hours.  Little  change  occurred  throughout 
the  duration  of  the  test;  no  further  feeding  was  observed  and  only  a  few  dead 
bees  and  larvae  were  noted.  The  material  was  obviously  strongly  repellent.  They 
fed  upon  the  calcium  arsenate,  however,  so  rapidly  and  to  such  effect  that  within 
an  hour  almost  the  entire  population  was  at  the  bottom  of  the  hive  in  a  dead 
or  dying  condition.  There  was  no  sign  of  repulsion  and  the  bees  fed  upon  the 
poisoned  syrup  at  least  as  readily  as  upon  plain  syrup.  So  sudden  and  complete 
was  the  killing,  that  no  additional  evidence  as  to  symptoms  of  arsenic  poisoning 
was  obtainable.     There  was  no  sign  of  recovery  of  any  of  the  bees. 

In  the  case  of  the  sulphur  dust,  this  was  consumed  quite  readily  by  the 
bees;  and,  far  from  there  being  any  evidence  of  repulsion,  it  seemed  to  be  more 
attractive  than  the  plain  syrup.  Droplets  exposed  on  the  tops  of  the  frames 
were  quickly  lapped  up,  following  which  the  bees  attempted  to  consume  the 
sulphur  residue.  There  was,  however,  no  pronounced  mortality  within  five 
hours  of  the  initiation  of  the  test;  but  by  9  a.m.  the  next  day  many  sick  and 
dead  bees  were  found  in  the  trap,  and  "  crawlers  "  were  numerous  in  the 
grass.  By  2  p.m.  the  bees  that  had  been  sick  had  gathered  in  bunches  and 
were  mostly  dead.  By  11  a.m.  on  the  third  day,  sick,  dying  and  dead  bees 
were  numerous  in  the  trap,  the  living  population  had  dwindled  to  a  very  few 
bees  and  some  of  the  larvae  had  apparently  crawled  out  of  the  cells. 

To  sum  up:  copper  sulphate,  lime  sulphur  and  nicotine  sulphate,  incorporated 
in  sugar  syrup  in  the  dilution  ordinarily  used  in  spraying,  and  fed  to  bees,  were 
so  strongly  repulsive  that  they  were  refused  by  the  bees  and,  as  a  result,  little 
poisoning  resulted.  Neither  calcium  arsenate  nor  sulphur  dust  gave  any  evi- 
dence of  repulsion  and  were  readily  consumed.  The  former  brought  about  the 
rapid  extinction  of  the  colony,  but  the  latter  was  much  slower  in  its  action. 

4.   STUDIES   FROM    COMMERCIAL    ORCHARDS 

From  time  to  time  reports  were  received  or  cases  were  noted  of  apparent 
conditions  of  poisoning  from  commercial  orchards.  In  such  cases,  samples  of 
dead  and  sick  bees,  together  with  pollen  and  nectar,  when  procurable,  were 
secured  and  subjected  to  chemical  analysis.  The  results  of  these  studies  are 
set  forth  in  the  following  pages. 

(a)  CHEMICAL    DATA   ON    SAMPLES    FROM    COMMERCIAL    ORCHARDS,    1928 

A  number  of  samples  obtained  from  commercial  orchards  during  the  investi- 
gation furnish  the  data  presented  in  the  following  table.  Though  complete 
analyses  wTere  made  in  most  cases,  only  the  most  significant  are  included  in 
the  table.  Furthermore,  the  results  of  many  analyses,  where  relevant  informa- 
tion is  lacking,  have  been  omitted  entirely.  Sufficient  typical  cases  have  been 
selected  to  give  a  fair  picture  of  the  situation  throughout. 


173 


TABLE  No.  34.— RESULTS  OF  EXAMINATION  OF  FIELD  SAMPLES,  1920-1930 


Orchard 
number 

Date 

Nature  of 
samples 

%  (As) 
Arsenic 

Ave.  As. 

per  bee 

(Internal) 

mg. 

1 

31/5/28 

4/6/28 
10/6/28 

20/6/28 

100  bees 

0-00023 

100     " 
100     " 

0-00023 
0-00043 

2 

3 

3  lots  of 
100  bees 

0-00029- 
0-00054 

4 

3/6/29 

100  bees 

0-00032 

5 

3/6/29 

74     " 

0-00027 

6 

30/5/30 
30/5/30 

75     " 
5  samples 
of  100  bees 

0-00006 
0  00010- 
0-00028 

7 

8 

10/6/32 
30/5/32 

100  bees 
100     " 

0-00045 
0-00015 

9 

10 

27/7/32 

100     " 

0-00033 

Remarks 


Orchard  sprayed  with  lime  sulphur-calcium  arsenate;  poisoning 
evident. 

While  neighbour  was  dusting  with  sulphur-lead  arsenate,  bees 
began  to  arrive  in  front  of  hive  in  dying  condition.  Severe 
poisoning  with  death  of  brood  followed. 

Very  sudden  and  severe  poisoning  resulting,  on  examination  of 
trie  two  colonies,  following  application  of  85-15  sulphur- 
lead  arsenate  to  neighbouring  orchard.  Pollen  from  comb 
analysed  -00452  %  arsenic;  sample  of  larvae  -00020  arsenic. 
Apples  not  in  bloom  but  wild  radish  in  bloom  in  dusted 
orchard. 

All  80  colonies  showed  severe  poisoning  of  adults  and  brood,  the 
two  strongest  most  severe.  All  neighbouring  orchards 
dusted  with  sulphur-lead  arsenate  and  after  each  applica- 
tion large  numbers  of  dead  and  dying  bees  appeared 
before  hives. 

All  7  colonies  suffered  severely,  two  being  completely  extermin- 
ated following  application  of  sulphur -lead  arsenate  to  all 
neighbouring  orchards.  Much  dead  brood  present  and 
pollen  samples  showed  -00237%  arsenic;  larvae  sample, 
•00237%  arsenic. 

Check  sample  of  unpoisoned  bees. 

Bees  placed  in  orchard  May  24,  before  bloom  appeared;  7-5  and 
8-6  hours  sunlight  on  May  24  and  25  respectively;  weather 
until  June  1  cold  and  wet,  temperature  only  once  reaching 
60°F.  Five  colonies  exterminated  by  June  1,  remaining  2 
slowly  died  out.  Poisoning  took  place  before  apple  bloom. 
Typical  of  conditions  in  1930  when  severe  poisoning  occurred 
in  nearly  all  apiaries  before  bloom. 

Poisoning  evident;  no  spraying  done  within  1  \  miles  of  apiary. 

One  colony  badly  poisoned;  only  spraying  practised  in  neigh- 
bourhood of  orchard. 

This  late  poisoning  was  apparently  due  to  a  neighbouring 
orchard  being  dusted  with  poisoned  Bordeaux  dust  for 
apple  maggot.  Wild  radish  in  bloom  in  the  orchard; 
mortality  large. 


(b)    CONCLUSION    FROM    EXAMINATION    OF    COMMERCIAL    SAMPLES    OF 

DEAD  BEES 

The  data  obtained  from  analyses  of  samples  of  dead  bees  showed,  in  general, 
ponderable  amounts  of  arsenic,  from  2  to  14  times  the  quantity  found  in  bees 
dying  from  natural  causes.  Poisoning  occurred  in  all  seasons,  but  was  most 
pronounced  following  periods  of  dull,  wet  weather.  Dead  brood,  taken  from 
frames  of  the  poisoned  colonies  also  gave  large  percentages  of  arsenic.  The 
results  indicate  that,  in  this  orchard  area,  practically  no  period  of  the  summer 
is  entirely  safe  for  hive  bees. 

(c)   GENERAL  SURVEY  OF  FIELD  CONDITIONS 

(i)  Fungicide  Dusts. — Orchards  treated  with  poisoned  copper  fungicides 
have  apparently  served  as  the  source  of  a  certain  amount  of  poisoning  among 
bees,  but  the  evidence  on  this  point  is  neither  so  clean-cut  nor  so  pronounced  as  in 
the  case  of  sulphur-lead  arsenate  dust  (90-10  or  85-15).  In  front  of  colonies 
poisoned  by  this  latter  mixture  it  was  quite  common  to  see  carpets  of  bees  6-40 
feet  in  length  and  of  varying  width.  The  bees  so  poisoned  are  unable  to  fly,  but 
crawl  or  bunch  on  the  ground  in  front  of  the  hives.  During  1929,  the  bloom 
came  out  very  suddenly  before  many  growers  had  applied  their  "  pink  "  applica- 
tions, resulting  in  much  spray  and  dust  being  applied  to  bloom.  We  have  records 
from  nine  lots  of  bees  placed  in  locations  widely  separated  from  each  other.  In 
four  of  these  areas  no  dusting  whatever  was  done.  No  poisoning  whatever 
occurred,  so  far  as  could  be  determined,  in  three  of  these  apiaries.  In  the  fourth, 
.some  poisoning  did  take  place,  but  not  of  a  type  to  be  noticed  by  the  casual 
observer.    In  another  orchard  where  spraying  had  been  practised,  but  not  during 


174 

bloom,  no  poisoning  was  evident  until  an  adjoining  orchard  was  dusted  with 
90-10  sulphur-lead  arsenate.  Dusting  was  completed  at  9  a.m.  Examination  at 
4.30  p.m.  revealed  many  "  crawlers  "  and  other  unmistakable  signs  of  poisoning, 
the  colonies  becoming  seriously  depleted  as  a  result. 


Fig.  70. —  (1)   Orchard  dusting:    (2)  showing  drift  of  dust  cloud  in  orchard 
(original  from  photo  by  C.  B.  Gooderham). 


Two  groups  of  colonies  were  placed  in  the  same  district  in  1929,  but  over 
a  mile  apart.  All  were  in  good  condition  up  to  May  30.  Owing  to  rain  and 
cold,  no  flight  occurred  in  either  apiary  until  June  5,  when  90-10  sulphur-lead 
arsenate  dust  was  applied  to  an  orchard  adjacent  to  one  of  these  apiaries. 
Crawlers  were  present  in  front  of  all  hives  in  this  apiary  and,  for  several  feet 
in  front  of  all  colonies  the  ground  was  covered  with  dead  bees,  and  there  was 
a  definite  break  in  brood  rearing.  Dead  bees  and  pollen  showed  a  high  content 
of  arsenic.  All  other  colonies  placed  in  areas  where  sulphur-lead  arsenate  was 
applied  suffered  severely,  many  were  killed  outright  and  only  with  the  greatest 
difficulty  could  the  remainder  be  built  up  for  overwintering. 

(ii)  Sulphur  Dust. — Unpoisoned  sulphur  dust  is  frequently  applied,  especi- 
ally in  bloom,  and  is  usually  accompanied  by  dysentery  among  the  bees.  In 
1929,  no  poison  dusting  was  carried  out  about  Gaspereaux,  though  considerable 
sulphur  dusting  occurred.  Dysentery  was  quite  pronounced,  clothing  hung  out 
to  dry  being  badly  spotted.  No  positive  evidence  of  serious  mortality  was 
obtained,  and  the  colonies  made  continuous  and  satisfactory  gains  for  several 


175 

weeks,  when  slight  poisoning  occurred  following  further  applications  of  poison 
dusts,  wild  radish  (Raphanus  Raphanistrum  L.)  being  then  in  bloom.  Feeding 
tests  indicate  that  considerable  mortality  may  be  occasioned  by  unpoisoned 
sulphur  dust,  but  not  comparable  to  that  caused  by  arsenicals. 

(iii)  Nicotine. — In  all  the  orchards  where  apiaries  have  been  placed,  we  have 
practised  nicotine  dusting  during  bloom,  in  order  to  eliminate  injury  from  suck- 
ing insects  such  as  the  green  apple  bug  (Lygus  communis  Knight)  to  the  bloom. 
The  applications  were  made  at  night  or  early  morning  when  no  bees  were  in 
the  tree.  A  strong  dust  containing  double  the  usual  strength,  i.e.  4  per  cent  of 
actual  nicotine,  was  employed.  Neither  in  these  nor  other  orchards  treated  with 
commercial  nicotine  dust  have  we  been  able  to  detect  signs  of  marked  poisoning, 
though  bees  were  noticed  freely  working  the  trees  and  occasionally  became 
dusted  with  lime.  It  would  appear  that  if  deaths  from  the  use  of  this  prepara- 
tion ever  occur  under  field  conditions,  they  must  be  too  few  to  be  detected  by 
ordinary  means.  It  has  already  been  noted  that  nicotine  dust  is  usually  applied 
when  the  bees  are  not  in  the  trees,  and  the  rapid  volatilization  of  the  nicotine 
would  remove  the  possibility  of  acute  poisoning. 

We  have  no  field  evidence  of  poisoning  from  nicotine  sprays,  and  since  this 
material  is  almost  invariably  used  in  combination  with  other  materials,  it  would 
be  difficult  to  isolate  the  action  of  this  one  ingredient.  It  is  not,  however,  con- 
sidered to  be  an  important  source  of  poisoning  under  field  conditions. 

(iv)  Arsenical  Sprays. — It  is  not  possible  to  distinguish  between  the  effects 
of  different  spray  mixtures  containing  arsenicals,  some  growers  using  poisoned 
Bordeaux  and  some  lime  sulphur  in  almost  every  community,  so  that  the  exact 
source  of  the  poisoning  could  not  be  determined  in  the  majority  of  cases.  As 
already  noted,  only  one  clear-cut  case  of  poisoning  from  sprays  occurred  during 
1929,  but  following  that  year  such  cases  became  increasingly  frequent. 

One  of  the  best  locations  for  making  observations  on  bee  poisoning  from 
sprays  was  Long  island,  where  no  dusting  had  been  practised  for  several  years 
and  where  bees  had  been  kept  under  close  observation  since  1928.  Bees  do  not 
appear  to  fly  across  the  two  miles  of  dyke  to  the  mainland  in  any  number  during 
bloom,  though,  of  course,  no  one  could  say  that  such  a  flight  never  occurs.  When 
the  mainland  orchards  are  blooming,  the  island  orchards  may  still  be  bare,  or 
nearly  so,  and,  under  such  conditions,  though  flight  might  occur,  we  have  failed 
to  demonstrate  it.  When  orchards  on  the  island  are  in  bloom  and  the  main- 
land largely  out  of  bloom  we  could  never  trace  the  bees  to  mainland  orchards. 

In  1928,  some  poisoning  occurred,  resulting  in  a  loss  of  the  field  force  which 
affected  the  performance  of  the  hives  very  materially.  Records  of  honey  pro- 
duced by  32  colonies  left  on  the  Island  during  the  season  of  1929  when  similar 
conditions  occurred,  and  32  others  kept  outside  during  bloom  and  only  taken  to 
the  island  for  the  clover  flow,  are  available.  The  two  lots  were  as  nearly 
similar  as  was  practical  to  select  them  and  all  other  conditions  were  equal.  The 
production  of  the  first  lot  was  2,200  pounds  of  honey,  of  the  second  3,100  pounds. 
Analysis  of  bees  from  the  first  lot  showed  large  amounts  of  arsenic  in  dead 
bees  and  excreta.  We  cannot  prove  that  the  bees  did  not  secure  this  poison  from 
dusted  areas  on  the  mainland,  but  it  is  noteworthy  that  colonies  used  from  the 
same  apiary  in  several  other  orchards,  in  which  spray  was  employed,  were  not 
poisoned.  Furthermore,  a  small  apiary  on  the  opposite  mainland  escaped 
noticeable  poisoning.  Moreover,  results  for  the  following  three  years  on  the 
same  location,  further  support  the  belief  that  the  poisoning  was  obtained  on  the 
island  as  a  result  of  sprays.  Poisoned  Bordeaux  was  the  spray  used  up  to  the 
time  poisoning  occurred.  It  should  be  said  that  there  was  little  evidence  of  dead 
brood,  there  being  no  colonies  killed  outright  and  no  definite  break  in  brood 
rearing  in  these  colonies  during  the  years  1928-1931  inclusive. 


176 

The  following  two  years  similar  poisoning  took  place.  The  season  of  1930 
was  characterized  by  several  days  of  cold  weather  in  the  pre-blossom  period  and 
during  early  bloom.  May  26  to  June  3,  was  generally  cool  with  the  ther- 
mometer not  going  above  62°  F.  at  any  time.  The  colonies  were  placed  in 
certain  orchards  on  May  24,  in  anticipation  of  immediate  bloom,  which,  how- 
ever, was  so  retarded  by  the  cold  weather  that  Gravenstein  was  not  in  full 
bloom  until  June  5.  There  was  rain  on  May  27,  28,  29,  31  and  on  June  1  and  2. 
On  June  3,  the  weather  cleared  and  became  warm  and  bright.  During  this 
period,  May  30  was  the  only  day  when  flight  was  possible,  there  being  7^  hours 
of  sunlight,  and  considerable  activity  was  noticed.  Poisoning,  in  many  cases 
severe,  immediately  developed.  A  number  of  colonies  were  wiped  out,  dead 
brood  developed,  and  all  colonies  were  so  weakened  that  they  were  removed 
from  the  orchards  as  being  useless  for  purposes  of  pollination. 

In  1931,  injury  was  particularly  severe,  which  may  at  least,  partially,  be 
accounted  for  by  the  fact  that  heavier  spraying  was  done  with  a  higher  arsenic 
content  than  ever  before.  Bees  from  a  small  apiary  on  the  mainland  worked 
the  bloom  in  orchards  immediately  opposite,  which  were  not  sprayed  as  heavily 
as  on  the  island.    These  bees  showed  no  poisoning. 

On  June  2,  1932,  about  100  colonies  of  bees  were  moved  to  Long  island  as 
Gravenstein  was  coming  into  bloom  and  careful  investigation  failed  to  yield 
any  evidence  that  the  bees  were  flying  to  the  mainland  in  any  numbers,  and 
there  is  much  evidence  that  they  did  not.  June  3  was  an  ideal  day  for  flight 
and  the  bees  worked  freely,  but  this  was  followed  by  five  days  of  cold,  damp 
and  changeable  weather  during  which  their  flight  was  inhibited,  while  on  the 
6th  and  7th  there  was  no  activity  whatever  in  the  bloom.  On  the  8th  there 
was  a  little  activity  and  on  the  9th  fairly  good  conditions.  Poisoning  was 
evident  on  June  4  and  increased  each  day  thereafter.  It  was  not  of  the  sudden 
acute  type  so  characteristic  of  poisoning  from  dust,  though  most  evident  after 
a  rain.  The  bees  at  the  east  end  of  the  Island  suffered  worst,  more  orchard 
and  heavier  spraying  being  done  in  that  territory.  The  bees  began  to  dwindle 
until  June  23,  when  a  careful  examination  showed  that,  in  order  to  save  the 
colonies,  drastic  uniting  would  have  to  be  carried  out  and,  accordingly,  the 
original  105  colonies  were  united  to  make  68  new  colonies,  some  of  these  con- 
sisting of  little  more  than  two  or  three  frames  covered  with  bees. 

The  foregoing  case  is  important  because  we  definitely  know  that  the  poison- 
ing was  caused  by  Bordeaux-arsenate  spray,  all  growers  on  the  Island  confining 
themselves  to  this  combination.  The  common  formula  used  was  3-10-40  plus 
one  pound  of  calcium  arsenate,  but  many  variations  were  used.  In  one  case 
some  Gravenstein  bloom  was  out  when  the  spraying  was  done,  which  was  on 
the  day  previous  to  the  colonies  being  introduced.  It  was  noticeable  that  bees 
in  this  orchard  suffered  worst,  though  all  colonies  showed  losses,  even  though 
no  spraying  had  been  done  for  five  days  previously.  The  accompanying  tables 
are  given  to  show  the  great  reduction  in  strength  as  a  result  of  this  poisoning. 


TABLE  No.  35.- 


177 

-STRENGTH  OF  COLONIES  MAY  11, 
POLLINATION  WORK* 


1932,  BEFORE  USING  IN 


Number 

of 
colony 

Combs 
covered 

Number 

of 
colony 

Combs 

covered 

Number 

of 
colony 

Combs 
covered 

Number 

of 
colony 

Combs 
covered 

238 

50 
6-0 
50 
50 
6-5 
60 
50 
90 
8-0 
50 
7-5 
6-0 
90 
60 
5-5 
50 
60 
50 
70 
60 
4-5 
60 
50 
100 
8-0 
9-5 
50 

267 
120 
264 
250 
273 
232 
279 
284 
257 
259 
77 
All 
211 
216 
201 
108 
240 
245 
224 
226 
123 
114 
260 
285 
159 
227 
288 

70 
9-5 
4-5 
8-0 
6-5 
60 
40 
5-5 
60 
60 
5-5 
8-0 
90 
60 
90 
50 
90 
6-5 
5-0 
5-5 
50 
4-5 
90 
50 
4-5 
70 
5-0 

206 

A15 

5-5 
8-0 
90 
4-0 
90 
4-5 
90 
50 
50 
50 
70 
4-5 
50 
70 
8-0 
60 
7-0 
4-5 
60 
90 
70 
50 
60 
7-0 
50 
8-0 
6-5 

87 
128 
132 
228 
117 
348 
210 
131 
126 
203 
200 
268 
261 
283 
277 
275 
124 
234 
255 
258 
265 
252 
218 
221 

7  5 

151 

30 
40 

34A 

154 

220 

39 

30 

214 

95 

100 

89... 

282 

100 

121  . 

237 

5-5 

78 

231 

50 

209 

B4.... 

6-5 

93... 

62 

7-5 

249... 

287 

50 

241 

266 

90 

153 

235 

50 

222 

230 

90 

88... 

102 

80 

158 

205 

60 

207 

248 

6-5 

45... 

155 

30 

225 

202 

90 

110  .. 

157 

5-5 

100    .. 

152... 

5-5 

213... 

262 

4-0 

80    . 

280 

5-5 

247... 

204 

60 

239... 

278 

233 

272... 

256... 

286 

"There  was  an  increase  in  the  strength  of  these  colonies  from  3  to  5  combs  from  May  11  to  May  26. 
The  latter  date  50  colonies  were  moved  to  the  Somerset  orchard,  where  no  poisoning  occurred  during  the 
6  days  they  remained.     On  June  2,  all  colonies  were  removed  to  Long  Island  and  left  there  until  June  10. 


The  following  tables  give  the  strength  of  the  colonies   on  June  23,   1932, 
after  having  been  used  in  the  pollination  work: — 

TABLE  No.  36 
A.  Colonies  United  Because  of  Weakness  Due  to  Poisoning 


Numbers  of  the  colonies 
united 

Number  of  combs 

covered  in  each 

colony  united 

Number  of  the  colonies 
united 

Number  of  combs 

covered  in  each 

colony  united 

278, 

207 

1          1          1 

234,       Dead 

264, 

209 

2           2 

211,           88, 

272 

1          1            \ 

248, 

241 

2          1 

158,         159, 

262 

1            \          \ 

226, 

B  4 

2           1 

218,         233 

W       H 

258, 

200, 

93 

2           1           1 

108,         250 

l!       11 

89, 

220, 

275 

H         1           1 

231,           39 

u     n 

240, 

255 

H       H 

102,           77, 

266 

2       \     2 

205, 

280, 

34A 

ii       1-1         i 

221,         132, 

252 

H       1        U 

151, 

230 

l          i 

A15,         225, 

2        1 

204, 

214, 

153 

2          1           1 

202,           80 

H       1* 

286, 

224, 

228 

oil 

100,         279, 

123 

i\      1        1 

258, 

128, 

114 

ii         ii           i 

-12               i-2                  2 

237,         235 

2        1 

201, 

265 

285,         131 

2        1 

95,* 

285 

8          1 

152, *      157 

5i        1 

284,* 

238 

4          4 

Queenless. 


60796—12 


178 

B.  Colonies  Left  Ununited 


Number 

ol 
colony 

Number 
of  combs 
covered 

Number 

of 
cofony 

Number 
of  combs 
covered 

Number 

of 
colony 

Number 
of  combs 
covered 

Number 

of 
colony 

Number 
of  combs 
covered 

121 

20 
20 
4-0 
6-0 
5-0 
4-0 
40 
8-0 
50 
5-0 

261 
277 
283 
110 
210 
259 
282 
277 
62 
216 

80 
8-0 

130 
50 
8-0 
7-5 
50 
50 
40 

11-0 

78 

30 
3-5 
2-5 
4-0 
40 
4-0 
8-0 
8-0 
7-0 
7-0 

45 
282 
227 
222 
126 
245 
203 
273 
131 

30 

34B 

155 

2-5 

117 

249 

40 

All 

213 

50 

206 

87 

3-5 

239 

247 

8-0 

120 

124 

154 

90 

256 

8-0 

260... 

287 

20 

288 

257 

We  have  many  records  to  show  that  orchards  may  be  sprayed,  even  when 
considerable  bloom  is  present,  with  the  different  commercial  mixtures  and  no 
losses  occur.  Indeed,  gains  are  frequently  recorded  in  such  cases;  but,  under 
certain  weather  conditions,  as  indicated  in  the  next  section,  severe  poisoning 
may  take  place.  Complete  loss  of  colonies  seldom  occurs  and  there  is  less  loss 
of  brood  than  is  found  in  most  cases  of  poisoning  from  sulphur-lead  arsenate 
dust. 

5.   INFLUENCE   OF   WEATHER   CONDITIONS   ON   BEE   POISONING 

One  would  expect  that  poisoning  would  be  greatest  in  prolonged  periods 
of  weather  favourable  for  flight  and  activity,  and  several  workers  have  definitely 
stated  that  this  is  the  case.  It  is,  therefore,  worthy  of  note  that  the  most  severe 
cases  recorded  were  those  which  occurred  following  periods  of  cold,  dull  or  wet 
weather,  during  which  the  bees  were  largely  or  entirely  confined  to  the  hives. 
With  the  coming  of  bright  sunny  weather  "  crawlers  "  would  suddenly  appear 
before  the  hives,  and  other  symptoms  of  poisoning  became  evident.  With  the 
clearing  of  the  weather  the  number  of  "  crawlers  "  would  gradually  diminish. 
Though  dwindling  of  brood  might  go  on  as  a  result  of  poison  stores,  no  cases  of 
new  poisoning  would  develop  until  another  period  of  dull  weather  when,  with 
the  return  of  flight,  the  condition  would  be  repeated  in  more  or  less  severity. 
Least  poisoning  was  noted  in  the  majority  of  cases  during  warm  bright  weather, 
even  though  poisons  were  applied  to  bloom. 

In  the  Lakeville  area  where  the  conditions  are  definitely  known,  some  inter- 
esting observations  were  made  in  1929.  All  colonies  were  in  good  condition  on 
May  30.  Rain  fell  from  6  p.m.  on  that  date,  causing  the  growers  to  apply 
a  dust,  some  the  same  night,  others  early  next  morning.  This  was  very  general 
all  over  the  area,  the  dusts  being  sulphur-lead  arsenate  90-10  and  85-15.  May 
31  to  June  2  was  cold  'and  foggy,  with  mist  and  rain.  On  June  2,  there  were 
brief  periods  of  sunlight  throughout  the  day.  The  most  severe  symptoms  of 
poisoning  immediately  became  evident,  and,  when  examined  next  day,  some 
of  the  colonies  were  already  practically  exterminated. 

In  1930,  a  number  of  colonies  were  set  aside  for  poison  tests  during  bloom, 
but  heavy  poisoning  occurred  before  bloom,  all  colonies  retained  for  the  purpose 
being  poisoned,  and  we  had  available  for  further  poison  tests  only  a  few  rented 
colonies  which  had  been  kept  outside  the  sprayed  area.  Two  were  placed  in  an 
orchard  which  was  heavily  dusted  with  sulphur,  one  in  an  orchard  dusted  with 
sulphur-lead  arsenate,  and  five  in  an  orchard  dusted  with  unpoisoned  Bordeaux 
dust  (12-88).  The  weather  turned  hot  and  bright  and  continued  thus  from 
June  3,  when  the  hives  were  placed,  until  June  11  when  they  were  removed.  Xo 
poisoning  occurred  even  in  the  orchard  treated  with  sulphur  and  arsenate.  This 
result  is  not  necessarily  conclusive,  since  a  single  colony  in  an  area  planted 
heavily  to  orchard  may  do  most  of  its  work  elsewhere,  but  at  least  it  is  con- 
sistent with  our  other  results. 


179 

Only  two  examples  will  be  given  from  1931.  Bees  were  not  placed  in 
orchards  until  bloom  was  well  started.  On  Long  island  bees  were  placed  on 
May  29,  the  two  preceding  days  being  favourable  for  flight.  No  spraying  had 
been  done  for  six  days  previous  to  that  date  anywhere  on  the  island.  May  31 
was  bright  and  warm  and  great  activity  was  noted  at  the  hives.  By  9  a.m. 
"  crawlers  "  and  clusters  of  sick  bees  were  numerous.  By  noon  most  of  the 
sick  bees  had  disappeared  and  few  were  apparent  the  following  day.  The  bees 
were  removed  to  the  South  Mountain,  out  of  the  poison  zone,  and  no  further 
poisoning  was  noted.  After  several  days  of  very  rainy  weather  typical  poison- 
ing symptoms  again  became  evident,  even  worse  than  before,  and  the  colonies 
continued  to  dwindle  for  some  time. 

At  Pereaux,  32  package  colonies  were  placed  in  an  orchard  May  25  at  9  a.m. 
The  day  was  cool  and  showery  when  the  colonies  were  placed.  Flight  was 
limited/  but,  within  an  hour  of  placing,  "  crawlers  "  were  observed  near  hives. 
At  5  p.m.  as  much  as  a  cupful  of  dead  bees  was  found  in  front  of  some  hives. 
Spraying  was  general  in  the  neighbourhood,  but  no  dusting  was  done  within  a 
mile.  No  spray  was  actually  on  the  apple  blossom  in  the  neighbouring  orchards, 
but  heavy  spray  was  observable  on  grass,  dandelions,  etc.  On  the  26th  it  was 
foggy  with  frequent  showers.  On  May  27  the  day  was  clear  and  bright.  There 
were  no  "  crawlers  "  and  apparently  no  further  poisoning. 

On  Long  island,  poisoning  was  noticed  in  apiaries,  particularly  at  the  west 
and  east  apiaries,  following  rain  on  June  2,  1930.  No  spraying  had  been  done 
for  six  days  previous  to  this  time,  but  most  orchards  had  been  sprayed  quite 
heavily.  Practically  all  orchardists  used  poisoned  Bordeaux,  but  most  of  them 
used  double  the  quantity  of  poison  recommended.  One  man  used  lime  sulphur- 
calcium  arsenate-aluminium  sulphate.  Bees  were  noticed  in  early  morning  in 
considerable  numbers  lapping  liquid  from  the  leaves.  In  the  afternoon  when 
trees  had  dried,  more  were  noticed  at  puddles.  Bees  were  observed  heavily 
working  on  trees  covered  with  Bordeaux. 

On  June  8  and  9  the  weather  was  cold  and  cloudy  with  some  rain.  Some 
bloom  still  remained  on  late  varieties  but  most  had  fallen;  spraying  was  general 
from  June  5.  Dead  bees  were  noted  on  this  date  in  large  numbers  in  front  of 
all  hives.  Numbers  of  sick  bees  were  noted  crawling  over  the  ground  in  the 
orchard  near  the  east  apiary,  which  was  newly  sprayed  with  lime  sulphur-iron 
sulphate  mixture.  The  herbage  was  also  covered  with  spray.  Crawling  bees 
were  also  found  in  a  pasture  northwest  of  the  apiary  where  blueberry  bloom  was 
abundant.  There  was  no  dusting  on  the  island  within  two  miles  of  the  apiary. 
It  has  already  been  fully  described  in  the  preceding  section  that  periods  of  bee 
poisoning  on  Long  island,  in  1932,  followed  periods  of  dull  wet  weather. 

Three  factors  may  account  for  these  results: — 

1.  Bees  after  confinement  for  several  days  appear  thirsty  and  seek  mois- 
ture on  leaves  and  elsewhere  with  great  activity,  including  that  from  leaves 
heavily  coated  with  poison.  Even  during  rains  bees  have  been  observed  making 
short  trips  for  water. 

2.  Pollen  supplies  being  depleted  during  confinement,  bees  actively  gather 
large  quantities  near  the  hives  at  the  first  opportunity. 

3.  During  periods  of  weather  favourable  for  flight  poisoned  pollen  may 
be  collected,  but  little  actually  consumed.  During  confinement  they  may  feed 
on  such  poisoned  stores,  the  sick  or  dead  bees  leaving  or  being  ejected  from 
the  hives  with  the  advent  of  favourable  weather. 

Nothing  in  this  section  should  be  interpreted  as  indicating  that  no  serious 
poisoning  ever  occurs  during  periods  of  fine  weather  which  is  certainly  not  the 
case.  The  fact  that  the  most  severe  cases  of  poisoning  have  occurred  following 
periods  of  dull  wet  weather,  seems,  however,  to  be  established. 

60706—  12i 


180 

6.   SOURCES   OF    BEE   POISONING    (FOOD) 

There  are  several  possible  sources  from  which  poison  may  be  obtained  by 
bees;  viz:  — 

(a)  Pollen 

(b)  Nectar 

(c)  Water 

(a)  POLLEN  AS  A  SOURCE  OF  BEE  POISONING 

Since  pollen  is  eaten  to  a  certain  extent  by  adult  bees,  as  well  as  fed  to  the 
brood,  it  would  be  expected  to  be  a  potent  source  of  poisoning  for  both  stages 
and  actual  observations  and  analyses  show  this  to  be  the  case.  The  following- 
table  shows  analyses  from  tent  experiments  and  field  samples  where  definite 
poisoning  occurred,  and  from  two  check  samples. 


TABLE  No. 


ARSENIC,  AS  METALLIC  As,  FOUND  IN  POLLEN 


No. 

Year 

1928 

2 

1928 

3 

1928 

4 

1928 

5 

1928 

6 

1929 

7 

1929 

8 

1929 

9 

1929 

10 

1929 

11 

1932 

12 

1932 

13 

1932 

14 

1932 

Source 


Lime  sulphur — calcium  arsenate 

Lime  sulphur — nicotine — calcium  arsenate 

Sulphur — lead  arsenate 

Field  sample  (probably  sulphur — lead  arsenate)... 
Field  sample  (probably  sulphur — lead  arsenate)  — 

Calcium  arsenate 

Bordeaux — calcium  arsenate 

Bordeaux — calcium  arsenate — nicotine  sulphate. 
Lime  sulphur — calcium  arsenate — nicotine  sulphate 

90-10 

Field  sample 

Field  sample 

Field  sample  (check) 

Field  sample  (check) 


Weight 

Arsenic 

(As) 

gms. 

p.c. 

1-5666 

•00116 

1-0208 

•00282 

1-129 

•00747 

2-9044 

•00069 

0-8720 

•00452 

1-2960 

•00111 

1-3245 

•00017 

1-7921 

•00063 

1-3870 

•00060 

1-9820 

•00057 

0-9142 

•00016 

0-8150 

•00019 

1-5390 

nil. 

0-9038 

nil. 

It  may  be  said  that  in  all  cases  where  severe  poisoning  occurred  accom- 
panied by  dead  brood,  pollen  analyses,  when  made,  invariably  showed  ponder- 
able amounts  of  arsenic.  That  the  pollen  secured  is  an  important  and,  in  many 
cases,  the  main  cause  of  the  poisoning  would  appear  evident.  It  seems  likely 
that  the  gradual  dwindling  away  of  a  poisoned  colony,  after  having  been  removed 
to  a  poison  free  locality,  may  be  attributed  largely  to  this  cause.  The  sudden 
increase  in  deaths  after  a  period  of  forced  confinement  may  have  a  similar 
origin. 

(6)  NECTAR  AS  A  SOURCE  OF  POISONING 

At  odd  times  throughout  the  course  of  this  project  samples  of  nectar  have 
been  collected  and  analysed.    The  percentages  found  arc  tabulated  as  follows:  — 

TABLE  No.  38.— ARSENIC,  AS  METALLIC  As,  FOUND  IN  ANALYSES  OF 

SAMPLES  OF  NECTAR 


No. 

Year 

Source 

Method  of  securing  sample 

Arsenic 
As 

1 

1928 
1929 
1931 

1931 

Field  sample 

Field  sample 

Apiary  Hive  No.  104 

Shaken  from  frames 

p.c. 
•00001 

2 

3 

Shaken  from  frames    

Pipetted  from  cells  in  frame 

•00003 

nil 

4 

Bee  Division,  Ottawa 

Shaken  from  frame 

Pipetted  from  cells 

.00005 

00006 

181 

The  small  amount  of  poison  found,  even  when  samples  were  secured  in  such 
a  way  as  not  to  preclude  the  possibility  of  contamination,  would  indicate  that 
nectar  is  not  an  important  source  of  brood  poisoning  which  is  evidently  derived 
mainly  from  other  sources.  Nevertheless,  since  young  larvae  may  be  very 
susceptible,  this  possibility  cannot  be  entirely  eliminated  without  further  infor- 
mation. 

(c)  WATER   AS   A   SOURCE   OF   POISONING 

Bees  are  frequently  observed  consuming  water  that  drips  from  limbs  or 
collects  in  drops  upon  leaves  covered  with  spray.  This  is  particularly  noticeable 
after  they  have  been  confined  several  days  by  dull  rainy  weather. 

In  this  connection  a  few  typical  quotations  from  the  literature  as  to  the 
use  that  bees  make  of  water  may  be  of  value.  Root  and  Root  (1929,  pp.  745-746) 
state  "  The  gathering  of  water  is  more  noticeable  in  the  period  of  early  spring 
brood  rearing  and  in  hot  weather  than  at  other  times."  Langstroth  and  Dadant 
(1927,  pp.  101-103)  state  "water  is  necessary  to  bees  to  dissolve  the  honey, 
which  sometimes  granulates  in  the  cells,  to  digest  pollen  and  to  prepare  the 
food  with  which  they  feed  the  larvae." 

"  Bees  take  advantage  of  any  warm  winter  day  to  bring  it  to  their  hives, 
and  in  the  early  spring  may  be  seen  busily  drinking  around  pumps,  drains,  and 
other  moist  places.    Later  in  the  season  they  sip  the  dew  from  grass  and  leaves." 

"  That  bees  cannot  raise  much  brood  without  water,  unless  they  have  fresh- 
gathered  honey,  has  been  known  from  the  times  of  Aristotle.  Buera,  of  Athens 
(Cotton,  p.  104)  said  in  1797,  '  Bees  daily  supply  the  worms  with  water;  should 
the  state  of  the  weather  be  such  as  to  prevent  the  bees  from  fetching  water  for 
a  few  days,  the  worms  would  perish.  These  dead  bees  are  removed  out  of  the 
hives  by  the  worker  bees  if  they  are  healthy  and  strong;  otherwise,  the  stock 
perishes  from  their  putrid  exhalations'." 

Phillips  (1928,  pp.  95,  135-136)  notes  "Water  is  needed  at  practically 
all  times  during  the  breeding  season,  perhaps  more  especially  in  hot  weather. 
The  bringing  of  water  to  the  hive  is  most  noticeable  in  the  early  spring." 

"  The  collection  of  water  by  field  bees  is  most  commonly  observed  in  early 
spring  and  during  the  hottest  parts  of  the  summer,  there  probably  being  less 
need  for  water  when  the  humidity  within  the  hive  is  high  because  of  the  eva- 
poration of  nectar.  Bees  have  been  known  to  collect  water  in  quantity  in 
extremely  hot  weather  and  to  place  it  on  the  bars  within  the  hive,  from  which 
places  it  disappears  by  evaporation,  thus  reducing  the  temperature  within  the 
hive." 

Langstroth  (1914,  pp.  293-294)  states  regarding  the  necessity  of  water  for 
bees  as  follows:  "It  is  absolutely  indispensable  when  they  are  building  comb 
or  raising  brood.  But  as  soon  as  the  grass  starts  and  the  trees  are  covered  with 
leaves  they  prefer  to  sip  the  dew  from  them.  As  soon  as  the  weather  becomes 
warm,  and  the  bees  can  supply  themselves  from  the  dew  on  grass  and  leaves  it 
will  not  be  worth  while  to  give  them  water  in  their  hives." 

Cowan  (1890,  p.  7)  remarks:  "Water  is  also  used,  but  it  is  not  stored, 
and  the  bees  only  collect  it  as  required." 

Our  own  observations  indicate  that  considerable  water  is  consumed  during 
apple  bloom.  Bees  have  been  noted  sipping  it  from  pools  in  the  orchard  after 
a  rain,  from  spray  covered  herbage  beneath  the  trees  and  from  the  foliage  and 
petals  of  the  apple  trees.  Even  in  very  dark  weather  and  during  light  rains, 
small  numbers  of  water-collectors  make  their  short  flights  from  and  to  the  hive. 
In  one  case  they  were  seen,  under  such  conditions,  freely  sipping  water  from  a 
spruce  hedge  adjacent  to  the  apiary.  The  foregoing  may  at  least  in  part  explain 
why  such  marked  poisoning  occurs  after  each  rain.  Drip  water  from  different 
situations  from  which  bees  were  observed  to  consume  it  was  collected  and 
analyses  performed.    The  results  are  presented  in  the  accompanying  table.    The 


182 

samples  of  water  were  pipetted  from  apple  petals,  leaves  and  from  blades  of 
grass  growing  beneath  the  trees.  The  liquid  samples  were  filtered  before 
analysis,  so  that  the  data  indicate  soluble  arsenic  or  copper,  or  the  colloidal 
state  of  these  metals. 

TABLE  No.  39.— ARSENIC,  AS  METALLIC  As,  IN  MG.  FOUND  IN  RAIN  WATER  FROM 
LEAVES,  ETC.,  IN  SPRAYED  AND  DUSTED  ORCHARD 


No. 

of 

sample 


As.  in  mg. 
per 
litre 


nil 


Copper  as 
metallic 

cop.  in  mg. 
per  litre 


0-7575 

1-0810 

1-7201 

8-5720 

15-3750 

18-9002 

1-8040 

None  taken 

nil 
Not  taken 


0  027 


Treatment  of  orchard 


Check. 

Sprayed  with  lime  sulphur — calcium  arsenate — aluminium  sulphate 
mixture.     Petals  and  leaves. 

Same  as  foregoing. 

Bordeaux,  8-35-160  used,  plus  6  lbs.  calcium  arsenate.     Heavy  rain 
all  morning  before  taking  sample. 

Lime  sulphur — calcium  arsenate — iron  sulphate  mixture  used.     Light 
rain  in  morning  before  taking  samples. 

Same  as  above,  but  in  different  part  of  orchard. 

Dusted  previous  day  with  sulphur — lead  arsenate   (90-10).     Heavy 
rain  fell  following  application. 

From  blades  of  grass  in  same  orchard  as  preceding. 

Sprayed  with  Bordeaux,  6-18-160  on  June  3  and  4.     Sample  taken 
June  6  following  heavy  downpour. 


7.   SOURCES   OF   BEE   POISONING    (PLANTS) 

It  has  sometimes  been  assumed  that  the  apple  or  other  fruit  bloom  is  the 
sole  source  of  poisoning,  but  the  role  of  other  flowering  plants  growing  in  or 
near  the  orchard  should  be  emphasized.  Poisoning  is  generally  first  noticed  with 
the  dandelion  bloom,  and  the  later  cases  of  serious  poison  are  usually  associated 
with  the  blossoms  of  the  wild  radish  (Raphanus  Raphanistrum  L.).  Both  of 
these  plants  are  very  common  about  all  the  orchards  and  both  of  them  are 
heavily  worked  by  bees. 

During  the  course  of  these  investigations  every  effort  has  been  made  to 
determine  the  source  of  cases  of  poisoning  occurring  after  and  before  bloom, 
and  in  most  cases  we  were  forced  to  the  conclusion  that  the  former  was  the 
source  of  the  pre-blossom  poisoning,  while  the  latter  accounted  for  most  post- 
blossom  cases.  In  a  few  cases  local  orchard  weeds,  as  ground  ivy  (Nepeta  huh  r- 
acea  (L.)  Trevisan)  or  adjacent  clover  fields,  may  be  responsible;  but  the 
former  two  are  evidently  the  only  two  species  of  plants  widely  involved  in  cases 
of  poisoning.  The  foregoing  does  not  include  those  cases  of  poisoning  that  may 
be  due  to  poisoned  water  obtained  from  spray  or  dust  covered  grass  or  herbage 
growing  beneath  treated  trees. 

Some  chemical  data  relating  to  this  subject  were  secured  in  1931  and  1932. 
Samples  were  taken  from  dandelion,  strawberry  blossoms,  rhododendron  and 
ground  ivy  in  sprayed,  dusted  and  untreated  orchards.  In  addition,  whole  flower 
heads  of  dandelion  were  removed  (with  a  razor)  just  above  the  receptacle.  In 
other  cases,  apple  stamens  just  below  the  anthers  were  similarly  removed.  In 
the  case  of  the  strawberry  blossom,  the  whole  flower  without  the  sepals  was 
taken  for  analysis.  Whole  flowers  from  rhododendron  and  ground  ivy  were 
employed  in  the  tests. 


183 


184 

TABLE  No.  40— ARSENIC,  AS  METALLIC  As,  FROM  DANDELION  AND  STRAWBERRY  BLOOM  AND 
STAMENS  FROM  DUSTED  AND  SPRAYED  ORCHARDS 


No. 


Date  of 
collection 


Source  of 
material 


Material 
used 


No.  in 

sample 


Arsenic,  as 
metallic  As. 


milligrams        p.c 


Remarks 


19 


28/31 
27/31 


27/31 


28/31 
27/31 
27/31 
28/31 
27/31 
28/31 
27/31 
28/31 
3 /6/32 


31/5/32 
3/6/32 
3/6/32 


2/6/32 
2/6/32 
3/6/32 
2/6/32 
2/6/32 


2/6/32 
3/6/32 
2/6/32 


2/6/32 


3/6/32 
3/6/32 
9/6/32 


Check 

Dusted  orchard. 

Sprayed  orchard 


Check 

Dusted  orchard 

Spraved  orchard. . . . 

Check 

Dusted  orchard  (1).. 
Dusted  orchard  (2).. 
Sprayed  orchard  (1). 
Sprayed  orchard  (2). 
Check 


Dandelion  heads 
Dandelion  heads 

Dandelion  heads 


Strawberry  bloom. 
Strawberry  bloom. 
Strawberry  bloom. 

Apple  stamens 

Apple  stamens 

Apple  stamens 

Apple  stamens 

Apple  stamens 

Dandelion  heads. . . 


•0002 
•0394 


1430 


12 

12  -0061 

(No  bloom  present) 


Sprayed . 
Sprayed. 


Dusted. 


Dandelion  heads. 
Dandelion  heads. 
Dandelion  heads. 


Check.. 
Check.. 
Dusted. . 
Check.. 
Sprayed. 


Strawberry  blossoms 
Strawberry  blossoms 
Strawberry  blossoms 
Rhododendron  bloom 
Ground  ivy 


Check.. 
Check.. 
Sprayed. 


Apple  stamens. 
Apple  stamens. 
Apple  stamens. 


Dusted. 


\pple  stamens. 


Dusted. 
Dusted. 
Dusted. 


Apple  stamens. 
Apple  stamens. 
Apple  stamens. 


25 


0-0007 
1-40 
0-0078 
0-0230 


nil 
0  0068 


0-0228 


nil 

•0044 
•0006 
•0009 
•0011 


Taken  from  untreated  area. 

Collected  while  orchard  was 
being  dusted  with  sulphur- 
lead  arsenate  (90-10j. 

Sprayed  with  "Wet-tex',' 
previous  day,  guaranteed 
to  contain  sulphur,  not  less 
than  56%;  lead  arsenate, 
not  less  than  18%;  other 
ingredients,  not.  over  26% 
No  rain  fell  between  appli- 
cation and  cutting  of 
samples. 

Same  data  as  No.  2. 


Same  data  as  No.  2. 
Same  data  as  No  3. 
Same  data  as  No.  3. 
All  cheek  material  collected 

from  unsprayed  areas,    or 

un dusted  areas. 
Bordeaux  8-35-160  plus  6  lbs. 

calcium  arsenate  used  on 

May  30. 
Bordeaux    4^-25-120    plus    5 

lbs.  calcium  arsenate  used 

on  May  30  and  31. 
85-15    sulphur-lead  arsenate 

used  at  rate  of  60  lbs.  per 

acre  on  June  2. 


nil 
nil 
•2150 


•0041 


•0017 
•0021 
•0016 


Same  data  as  No.  13. 

Wettable  sulphur  containing 
6  lbs.  of  calcium  arsenate 
to  160  gallon  tank. 


Lime-sulphur-aluminium 
sulphate  mixture  plus  2  lbs. 
calcium  arsenate  per  100 
gals. 

85-15  sulphur-lead  arsenate 
applied  previous  day  fol- 
lowed by  heavy  rain. 

Same  data  as  No.  13. 

Same  data  as  Xo.  13. 

Dusted  previous  day  with 
sulphur-lead  arsenate  (90- 
10).  Heavy  rain  followed 
application. 


The  fact  that  samples  from  both  dusted  and  sprayed  orchards  show  arsenic 
present,  in  quantity  likely  to  cause  trouble  if  gathered  by  bees,  is  apparent.  The 
total  arsenic  taken  from  whole  dandelion  heads  was  generally  greater  in  the 
sprayed  than  in  the  dusted  orchard,  sometimes  very  much  greater.  The  stamens, 
on  the  other  hand,  especially  the  first  collection  in  1931,  sometimes  show  more 
arsenic  from  the  dusted  trees.  The  analysis  of  the  second  collection  shows  a 
decidedly  smaller  quantity  of  arsenic  than  the  first — this  may  be  duv  to  the 
wind  shaking  off  the  sulphur  dust,  or  the  uneven  dusting.  Rain  falling  after 
application  evidently  removes  dust  more  readily  than  spray. 

The  difference  in  the  character  of  the  spray  deposit  is  evidently  more 
important  than  the  actual  amount  present.  The  dust  is  loosely  exposed  and 
readily  gathered  by  the  body  hairs  and  stored  in  the  pollen.  The  spray  forms 
a  film  less  readily  dislodged  and  less  likely,  after  drying,  to  be  removed  by 
bees,  except  as  it  becomes  dissolved  in  rain  or  dewr,  when  it  may  be  imbibed, 
or  the  film  may  be  softened  and  lapped  up  by  the  bees.  This  would  help  to 
explain  why  wre  do  get  serious  injury  from  spray  following  a  rain. 


185 


:    *SXa 


186 


8.  INFLUENCE  OF  COLONY  SIZE  AND  APIARY  SIZE  ON  BEE 

POISONING 

Large  colonies,  because  of  their  greater  field  force,  suffer  worse,  proportion- 
ately, than  weak  colonies  or  package  bees,  as  a  general  rule.  Similarly,  in  large 
apiaries  our  observations  indicate,  on  the  whole,  greater  losses.  This  may  be 
because  the  bees  from  small  apiaries  are  not  obliged  to  work  the  whole  area 
intensively. 

9.   INFLUENCE   OF   TIME   OF   PLACING   IN    THE   ORCHARD 

The  period  for  bee  poisoning  extends  from  some  time  before  the  apple 
comes  into  bloom  until  some  time  thereafter,  and  where  poison  sprays  for  apple 
maggot  are  applied  it  may  extend  into  August.  It  is  frequently  more  severe 
shortly  before  and  shortly  after  the  bloom  than  during  that  period,  partly 
because  more  spraying  or  dusting  is  done  at  that  time  and  partly  because  less 
bloom  is  then  present  and  the  bees  concentrate  on  bloom  growing  in  and  near 
the  orchards.  In  most  districts,  little  poison  is  applied  during  the  period  of  full 
bloom.  Some  of  the  early  bloom  usually  gets  sprayed,  as  well  as  some  of  the 
late  bloom.  Spraying  or  dusting  during  bloom,  when  practised,  is  usually  though 
unfortunately  not  always,  with  a  fungicide  only.  Only  a  small  proportion  of 
growers  use  poison  in  the  dust  during  bloom.  At  this  period  bloom  is  abundant 
and  the  bees  range  widely.  Poisoning  may  occur  during  bloom  as  a  result  of 
pre-blossom  sprays,  especially  under  certain  weather  conditions,  as  already 
explained. 

The  foregoing  will  clearly  indicate  that  when  bees  are  to  be  placed  in  the 
orchard  for  pollinizing  purposes,  it  is  advisable,  from  all  standpoints,  to  do  so 
only  after  the  early  varieties  are  in  bloom,  and  to  remove  them  before  the  calyx 
application  is  made.  Even  this  may  not  avoid  injury  under  all  conditions,  but 
it  will,  at  least,  reduce  it  to  a  minimum. 

10.    EVIDENCE    OF    POISONING   AMONG   WILD    BEES 

From  the  very  nature  of  things,  field  evidence  of  poisoning  among  wild  bees 
is  very  hard  to  obtain.  Curiously  enough,  they  may  be  found  nesting  in  great 
numbers  in  areas  where  the  most  severe  losses  of  hive  bees  have  taken  place. 

Dead  brood  could  rarely  be  found  in  the  nests,  and  dead  bees  never  occurred 
in  large  enough  numbers  in  nests  to  indicate  poisoning,  though  large  numbers 
may  succumb  as  a  result  of  drowning  within  the  nest.  It  may  be  that  such 
bees  never  reach  the  nest,  but  die  in  the  orchard  and  so  cannot  be  detected. 

The  result  of  analysis  of  pollen  from  districts  where  dusting  is  generally 
practised  is  therefore  of  interest. 

TABLE  No.  41— ARSENIC   (As)    FOUND  IN  POLLEN,  FROM   NESTS  OF  WILD  BEES 


Source 

Date 

Weight 

Arsenic 

Species 

No.    1 — Lakeville 

20-6-29 
22-6-29 
22-6-29 
23-6-29 
5-7-30 
9-7-30 
10-7-30 
15-7-30 
19-7-30 
18-7-30 
18-7-30 
18-7-30 
18-7-30 
21-7-30 
24-7-30 

gms. 

p.c. 
00031 

0  0005 
0  0019 
0  0017 
0  0018 
0  0002 
0  0003 
0  0003 
0  0026 
0  0002 

nil 

nil 
0  0006 

nil 
0  0002 

//.  sin  Hoc  inn 

No.    3— Woodville 

No.    4 — Lakeville. . . 

•  • 

No.    6— Woodville 

- 

No.    1 — Long  island 

01270 
10900 
1-3830 
0-7780 
0  0890 
2-4454 
0  0980 
01234 
0-4030 
0  0747 
0-9720 

M 

No.    2 — Long  island 

//.  cniterus 

No.    3 — Long  island 

II .  8  mi  lac  in  a  . 

No.    6 — Centreville 

No.    7 — Lakeville 

II.  smilacina 

No.    8 — Centreville 

II .  arctuitus 

No.    9 — Centreville 

No.  10 — Centreville 

II .  smilacina 

No.  11 — Centreville 

No.  12— Blomidon  (Shore)... 

No.  13— Welsford 

II.  smilacince 

187 


;$•   ■ 

mh' 

!  ,-,*:; 

1     *-; 

'♦  ' ' 

t 

■■*&.  . 

1    ♦* 

'p*>j* 


♦Vj* 


'- 


188 

The  fact  that  such  a  large  proportion  of  the  pellets,  collected  at  random, 
contain  measurable  amounts  of  arsenic  would  lead  one  to  suppose  that  poison- 
ing among  wild  bees  should  be  common;  but  if  so,  it  is  difficult  to  demonstrate, 
and  over  the  period  studied  we  have  not  been  able  to  detect  any  diminution  in 
the  effective  population  traceable  with  certainty  to  this  cause.  Much  more 
careful  studies  over  a  longer  period  would  be  necessary  before  we  could  speak 
with  confidence  on  this  point. 

11.   SUMMARY   OF   POISON   TESTS 

(a)  The  question  of  poisoning  of  bees  from  poison  sprays  and  dusts  used  in 
orchards  is  obviously  important  from  the  standpoint  of  their  utilization  for 
pollination  purposes.  A  survey  of  the  situation  shows  that  the  hive  bee  popula- 
tion has  been  greatly  depleted  during  recent  years  and  the  evidence  points 
clearly  to  the  conclusion  that  this  has  been  directly  due  to  the  use  of  poison 
sprays  and  dusts.  With  the  advent  of  dusting,  many  apiaries  were  completely 
wiped  out,  so  that  no  large  commercial  apiaries,  except  those  that  were  removed 
during  the  danger  period,  remained.  The  growing  practice  of  using  larger 
spray  outfits,  resulting  in  heavier  applications,  and  a  different  type  of  spray 
nozzle  resulting  in  greater  drift  to  surrounding  vegetation,  has  increased  the 
incidence  of  poisoning  from  this  cause. 

(6)  The  most  important  fungicides  used  are  (1)  lime  sulphur  in  various 
combinations  using  calcium  or  lead  arsenate  as  a  poisoning  ingredient;  (2) 
copper  sulphate  in  the  form  of  Bordeaux  mixture  or  copper-lime  dust,  to  which 
one  of  the  foregoing  poisons  has  been  added;  (3)  sulphur  dust  usually  in  com- 
bination with  lead  arsenate,  and  (4)  nicotine  sulphate  in  liquid  form  or  in  the 
form  of  a  "  contact  dust,"  with  hydrated  lime  as  a  filler.  Under  orchard  con- 
ditions it  would  appear  that  arsenic  in  the  form  of  lead  or  calcium  arsenate  is 
the  main  source  of  bee  poisoning  in  these  mixtures;  but  even  sulphur  dust  alone 
may  cause  trouble,  though  not  comparable  to  that  caused  by  arsenicals.  Clear 
evidence  of  repulsion  from  copper  sulphate,  lime  sulphur  and  nicotine  is  obtained 
from  tests  under  controlled  conditions;  but  under  orchard  conditions  this  repul- 
sion appears  to  be  temporary  and  does  not  prevent  serious  losses  from  occurring. 

(c)  Arsenical  poisoning  results  in  partial  to  complete  paralysis  and  is  first 
evidenced  by  "crawlers"  appearing  in  front  of  the  hive;  bees  come  together 
in  bunches;  the  abdomen  is  distended  and  severe  dysentery  makes  its  appear- 
ance, followed  by  death  of  adult  bees.  This  is  soon  followed  by  death  of  larvae 
and  pupae.  Symptoms  of  sulphur  poisoning  are  similar  but  less  severe.  Clear 
cases  of  copper  or  nicotine  poisoning  under  field  conditions  were  not  obtained. 

(d)  When  the  internal  arsenic  in  bees  is  greater  than  -00004  mg.  of  metallic 
arsenic  per  bee,  poisoning  may  be  expected,  definitely  so.  when  higher  than 
•0008  mg.  is  detected. 

(e)  Field  observations,  supplemented  by  chemical  analyses  of  numerous 
samples,  lead  to  the  following  conclusions:  — 

(i)  All  sprays  containing  an  arsenical  as  an  ingredient  were  dangerous 
when  applied  during  bloom. 

(ii)   Only  sprays  or  du^ts  containing  arsenicals  resulted  in  dead  brood. 

(iii)  The  combination  causing  the  most  sudden  and  complete  mortality 
was  sulphur-lead  arsenate  in  dust  form. 

(iv)  In  general,  less  mortality  resulted  from  sprays  and  dusts  containing 
copper  or  nicotine  as  an  ingredient  along  with  the  arsenical. 

(v)  Sulphur  dust  free  from  poison  may  cause  death  oi  bees,  but  to  what 
extent  this  occurs  under  field  conditions  is  not  clear.  Evidently  it  is 
much  less  deadly  than  arsenicals,  besides  bein^r  less  sudden  and  com- 
plete in  its  action. 


189 

(vi)  Field  observations,  supplemented  by  chemical  analyses  indicated  that 
the  greatest  poisoning  usually  occurs  just  previous  to  and  just  after  the 
main  bloom.  Nevertheless,  no  time  during  the  season  from  May  until 
August  is  completely  safe.  Sprays  for  apple  maggot  applied  as  late  as 
August  sometimes  cause  severe  poisoning. 

(/)  Weather  conditions  have  an  important  bearing  on  the  incidence  of 
poisoning  among  bees.  Very  severe  poisoning  has  been  noted  even  when  pre- 
vailing conditions  are  cool  and  wet,  and  some  of  the  worst  cases  have  taken  place 
following  brief  bursts  of  fine  weather  intervening  between  periods  of  broken 
weather.  On  the  other  hand,  attempts  to  secure  poisoning  by  placing  a  few 
hives  in  an  orchard,  sprayed  or  dusted  with  the  most  deadly  mixtures  when  the 
weather  was  optimum  for  flight,  have  sometimes  resulted  in  failure.  Several 
factors  may  account  for  this: — 

(i)  Bees  after  several  days  confinement  greedily  seek  moisture  from 
poisoned  leaves,  petals  and  poison-covered  herbage  growing  beneath 
trees,  which  analyses  show  to  contain  large  quantities  of  arsenic, 
(ii)  During  confinement,  supplies  are  depleted  and  at  the  first  opportunity 
bees  are  very  active  in  collecting  new  stores  near  at  hand,  resulting  in 
much  poisoned  pollen  being  brought  in. 
(iii)  During  periods  of  weather  favourable  for  flight,  poisoned  pollen  may 
be  collected  but  not  consumed,  and  this  may  be  fed  upon  during  periods 
of  confinement. 

(g)  The  main  source  of  poisoning  of  bees  and  brood  is  evidently  pollen, 
but,  under  certain  conditions,  drop  water  from  sprayed  leaves,  petals  or  herbage 
growing  in  the  orchard  may  be  a  very  important  factor.  Some  writers  mention 
nectar,  but  our  analyses  show  either  no  poison  in  the  nectar  or  only  minute 
amounts,  and  the  possibility  that  this  may  be  due  to  contamination  in  gather- 
ing the  sample  is  not  entirely  excluded  in  some  cases.  Nevertheless,  even  the 
minute  quantities  detected  may  be  deadly  to  very  young  larvae. 

(h)  It  is  often  assumed  that  poison  applied  to  the  fruit  bloom  is  the  chief 
or  sole  cause  of  loss,  but  this  is  not  the  case.  Severe  cases  of  poisoning  before 
and  during  bloom  are  sometimes  attributable  to  poison  obtained  from  dande- 
lion bloom  growing  in  or  near  the  orchard.  Later  cases  of  poisoning  were  mainly 
traceable  to  wild  radish ;  but  many  other  plants  may  serve  as  sources  of  poison- 
ing due  to  the  drip  or  drift  of  poisoned  sprays  or  dusts. 

(i)  It  was  noticed  that  large  colonies  and  large  apiaries  often  exhibited 
the  most  severe  poisoning. 

(j)  Least  trouble  was  experienced  when  bees  were  not  placed  in  the  orchards 
until  the  early  varieties  were  in  bloom  after  the  application  of  the  "  pink  "  spray 
and  taken  away  before  the  beginning  of  the  "  calyx "  spray.  For  reasons 
already  given,  however,  this  did  not  eliminate  all  cases  of  poisoning,  though 
it  reduced  them  to  a  minimum.  It  is  impossible,  however,  to  maintain  apiaries 
anywhere  in  the  entire  fruit  belt  at  any  time  from  May  until  August,  without 
incurring  some  risk  of  loss. 

(k)  Samples  of  pollen  taken  from  the  nests  of  solitary  bees  showed  ponder- 
able amounts  of  arsenic,  more  than  enough  to  destroy  the  larvae  of  hive  bees. 
Evidence  of  depletion  of  the  solitary  bee  population  from  this  cause  is  difficult 
to  secure  and  requires  further  observation. 


VI.  LITERATURE  CITED 

AUCHTER,   E.    C. 

1921.     Apple  pollen  and  pollination  studies  in  Maryland.     Proc.  Amer.  Soc.  Hort.  Sci. 
18:51-80. 
Ballard,  W.  R. 

1916.     Methods  and  problems  in  pear  and  apple  breeding.    Md.  Ag.  Exp.  Sta.  Bui.  196:88. 
Barclay,  R.  A. 

1928.  Use  of  bees  for  pollination  in  N.J.    Rural  N.Y.  87:478. 
Bercaw,  G.  W. 

1924.    Fruit  bloom  and  bees.     Cal.  Cultivator.    62:285. 
Bertholf,  L.  M. 

1931.    Reactions  of  the  honey-bee  to  light.    Jour.  Agr.  Res.  42:379-419. 

1931a.  The    distribution    of   stimulative    efficiency    in    the  ultra-violet   spectrum    for    the 
honey-bee.    Jour.  Agr.  Res.  43:703-713.     (Author's  summary.) 
Bonnier,  Gaston 

1906.     Sur  la  division  du  travail  chez  les  abeilles.    Comp.  Rend.  Acad.  Sci.  143:941-946. 
Borchert,  A. 

1930.  Berliner    tierarztliche    Wochensehrift,    46:84.      (Abstracts   in    Bee    World,    10:132; 
11:47  and  Amer.  Bee  Journal,  70.327.) 

Bourne,  A.  I. 

1927.    The    poisoning    of    honey-bees    by    orchard    sprays.     Mass.    Agr.    Exp.    Sta.    Bui. 
234:73-84. 
Britton,  W.  E.  and  Viereck,  H.  L. 

1906.    Insects   collected   from    flowers   of   fruit   trees   and   plants.    Conn.   Agr.   Exp.   Sta. 
Ann.  Rpt,  1905:207-221. 
Burrell,  A.  B.  and  King,  George  E. 

1931.  A   device  to   facilitate   pollen   distribution  by  bees.    Proc.   Amer.  Soc.   Hort.   Sci. 
28:85.     (Copy.) 

Chittenden,  F.  J. 

1926.  Summary  of  20  years  study  at   Rov.  Hort.  Soc.  Gdns.,   Mem.  Hort.  Soc.   N.Y. 
3:79-85. 

Coke,  J. 

1931.    The    use    of    sDrays,    dusts,    and    fertilizers    on    apple    orchards    in    the    Annapolis 
valley,  Nova  Scotia.    Economic  Annalist,  1:5-6. 
Corrie,  L.  G. 

1916.    Experiments    on    the    pollination    of    fruit    trees.    Jour,    of    Heredity.    7:365-369. 
Cowan,  T.  W. 

1890.     "  The  Honey  Bee,"  pg.  7. 
Crane,  J.  E. 

1924.  Factors  influencing  nectar  secretion.     Gleanings   in  Bee  Cult.  52:707-708. 
Crane,  M.  B. 

1927.  Studies   in   relation    to    sterility    in    plums    and    cherries,    apples   and   raspberries. 
Intermit,  con.  on  flowers  and  fruit  sterility.     Memoirs  Hort.  Soc.  X.Y.  3:119-134. 

Crane,  M.  B.  and  Lawrence,  W.  J.  C. 

1929.  Genetical    and    cytological    aspects   of   incompatibility   and   sterility   in   cultivated 
fruits.    Jour.  Pom.  and  Hort.  Sci.  7:284-301. 

1930.  Fertility  and  vigour  of  apples  in  relation  to  chromosome  number.    Jour.  Genetics. 
22:153-163. 

Darlington,  C.  D.  and  Moffett.  A.  A. 

1926.    Honey-bees  as  an  aid  in  fruit  growing.    Am.  Fruit  Grower.    46:12. 

1'930.    Primary  and  secondary  chromosome  balance  in  Pyrus.    Jour.  Genetics.    22:129-151. 
Davis,  J.  J. 

1926.     Honeybees  as  an  aid  in  fruit    growing.     Amer.  Fruit  Grower.     46:12. 
Demuth,    George   S. 

1912.    Comb  honey.    U.S.  Dept.   Agr.  Farmers'  Bui.  503. 

1.922.    Bee-keeping  and  agriculture.     Gleanings  from  Bee   Culture.     50:229-33. 
DeOng,  E.  R. 

1925.  Honey-bee    as   a    pollinizer.     Cal.    Agr.    Exp.    Sta.    Circ.   297:17-22. 

190 


191 

Detjen,  L.  R. 

1926.  Physiological   dropping    of   fruit.    Univ.   of   Del.   Agr.   Exp.   Sta.   Bui.    143    Tech 
No.   6. 

Doane.   R.   W. 

1923.    Bee-keeping   vs.   spraying.    Jour.   Econ.   Ent.    16:527-31. 
Dyce,  E.  J. 

1929.  Bees   help    the    fruit   grower.    The    Canadian   Hort.   52:5. 
Einset,  O. 

1930.  Cross-unfruitfulness  in  the  apple.    N.Y.  State  Exp.  Sta.  Tech.  Bui.  159. 
Farrar,  C.  L. 

1929.     Bees  and  apple  pollination.     Mass.  Agr.  Exp.  Sta.,  Special   Circ.  7. 

1931.  The  evaluation  of  bees  for  pollination.    Jour.  Ec.  Ent.  24:622-627. 
Filmer,  R.  S. 

1932.  Brood  area  and  colony  size  as  a  factor  in  activity  of  pollination  units.     Jour. 
Ec.  Ent.  25:   2  pp.  336-343. 

Frisch,  Karl  von 

1923.     Die  "  Sprache"  der  Bienen.    Zool.  Jahrb.,  Zool.  u.  physiol.     40:1-186. 

1923-24.    The  colour  vision  of  bees  and  the  colour  of  blossoms.     Amer.  Bee  Jour.  63:458. 
(Translated  by  Geo.  E.  King). 
Gates,  D.  N. 

1917.     Honey  bees  in  relation  to  horticulture.     Mass.  Hort.  Soc.  Trans.  1 :71-88. 
Gowen,   J.    W. 

1920.  Self-sterility  and  cross-sterility  in  the  apple.    Maine  Ag.  Exp.  Sta.  Bui.  287:61-88 
Hedrick,  M.  P.  and  Wellington,  R. 

1912.     An  experiment  in  breeding  apples.     N.Y.  State  Agr.  Exp.  Sta.  Bui.  350. 
Hendrickson,  A.  N. 

1927.  Bees  as  pollen  distributors.    Am.  Bee.  Jour.  67:250-2. 
Hilgendorff.  O.  and  Borchert,  A. 

1926.    Uber  die  Empfindlichkeit  der  Bienen  gegen   Arsenstanbemittel.     Nachrichtembl. 
F.  D.  Deutsch  Pflanzenschutzdienst.    Bd.  6  s.  37-38. 
Hockey,  J.  F.  and  Harrison,  K.  A. 

1930.  Apple  pollen  may  be  wind-borne.    Iowa  St.  Hort,  Soc.  Rep.  65:248-250. 
Hooker,  H.  D. 

1922.     Certain  responses  of  apple  trees  to  nitrogen  applications  oi  different  kinds  and  at 
different  seasons.    Univ.  Missouri  Agr.  Exp.  Sta.  Res.  Bui.  50:1-18. 

1925.  Annual  and  biennial  bearing  in  N.  Y.  apples.     Miss.  State  Res.  Bui.  75:3-16. 
Hooper,  Cecil  H. 

1921.  Pollination  of  fruits.     Jour,  of  Ministry  of  Agr.  28. 

1929.    The  study  of  the  order  of  flowering  and  pollination  of  fruit  blossoms  applied  to 
commercial  fruit  growing.     Jour.  Royal   Soc.   Arts.    77:424. 

1931.  Insect  visitors  to  fruit  blossoms.    J.  South-Eastern  Agr.  Col.,  Wye.,  Kent.,  28:211- 
£,  bibl.  9. 

HOW  LETT,    F.    S. 

1926.  Methods  of  procedure  in  pollination  studies.     Proc.  Amer.  Soc.  Hort.  Sci.     23: 
107-119. 

Some  factors  of  importance  in  fruit  setting  studies  with  apple  varieties.    Proc. 
Amer.  Soc.  Hort.  Sci.  23:307-315. 

1927.  Apple   pollination   studies  in   Ohio.    Ohio   Agr.   Exp.  Sta.  Bui.   404. 
Apple  varieties  and  fruit  setting  factors.    Am.  Fruit  Growers.    47:7. 

1927.  Further    self-    and    cross-pollination    studies    with    Baldwin    apple.      Proc.    Amer. 
Soc.  Hort.  Sci.  Volume  24  p.  105. 

1929.    Further   experiments    on    the    relative    self-fruitfulness    of    apple    varieties.     Proc. 

Amer.  Soc.  Hort.  Sci.  26:49. 
193H.    Horticultural  investigations  at  the  Ohio  Station.    Ohio  Agr.  Exp.  Sta.  Bui.  470. 
Hutson,  R. 

1926.  Relation  of  the  honey-bee  to  fruit  pollination  in  N.J.    N.J.  Agr.  Exp.  St.  Bui. 
434:1-32. 

1928.  Package  versus  overwintered  bees  for  orchard  use  as  pollinizers.    Amer.  Bee  Jour. 
March,   p.   128. 

Knowlton,  H.  E. 

1929.  Some    recent    results    in    apple    sterility    studies.      Amer.    Soc.    Hort.    Sci.    Proc. 
26:62-64.    Abs.  in  W.  Va.  Agr.  Exp.  Sta.  Bui.  244:54. 

Kobel,  F. 

1930.  Die   verschiedenen   Formen    der   Sterilitat   bei    unseren    Obstgewachsen.     Viertel- 
jahrsschrift  der  Naturforschenden  Gesellschaft  in  Zurich.     75:56-160. 

Langstroth.  L.  I.  and  Dadant. 

1927.  The  hive  and  the  honey-bee.  pp.  101-103. 


192 

Latimer,   L.   P. 

1931.     Further  observations  on  factors  affecting  fruit  =etting  of  the  Mcintosh  apple  in 
New  Hampshire.     Pro.  Amer.  Soc.  Hort.  Sci.  28:87. 
Lewis.  C.  I.  and  Vincent.  C.  C. 

1909.     Pollination  of  the   apple.     Oreg.  Agr.   Exp.  Sta.  Bui.   104:3-40. 
Lundie.  A.  E. 

1925.  The  flight  activities  of  the  honey-bee.     U.S.D.A.  Bui.  No.  1328. 
1927.    Honey-bee  and  the  fruit  grower.    Farming  in  S.  Africa.     1:384-7. 

MCCULLOCH,    J.    W. 

1914.    The  relation  of  the  honey-bee  to  other  insects  in  cross-pollination  of  the  apple 
blossom.    Kansas  State  Hort.  Soc.  46th  Ann.  Meeting,  pp.  85-88. 
MacDaniels,  L.  H. 

1927.  An  evaluation  of  certain  methods  used  in  the  study  of  the  pollination  require- 
ments of  orchard  fruits.     Mem.  Hort.  Soc.  N.Y.  3:139-150. 

1928.  Pollination  studies  in  N.Y.  State.    Proc.  Am.  Soc.  Hort.  Sci.    25:129-137. 

1930.  The  possibilities  of  hand  pollination  in  the  orchard  on  a  commercial  scale.  Proc. 
Amer.  Soc.  Hort.   Sc.  27:370-3. 

1931.  Further  experience  with  the  pollination  problem.  N.Y.  State  Hort.  Soc.  Proc. 
76:32-37. 

1931(a)  Practical  aspects  of  the  pollination  problems.     Am.  Bee  Jour.  71:116-17. 
MacDaniels,  L.  H.  and  Furr,  J.  R 

1930.  The  effect  of  dusting-sulphur  upon  the  germination  of  the  pollen  and  the  set  of 
fruit  of  the  apple.     Cornell  Univ.  Agr.  Exp.  Sta.  Bui.  499. 

MacDaniels,  L.  H.  and  Heinicke,  A.  J. 

1929.  Pollination  and  other  factors  affecting  the  set  of  fruit.  N.Y.  Cornell  Agr.  Exp. 
Sta.  Bui.  497. 

McIndoo.  F.  E.  and  Demuth,  G.  H. 

1926.  Effects  on  honey-bee  of  spraying  fruit  trees  with  arsenicals.    U.S.  D.A.  Bui.  1364. 
Macoun,  W.  T. 

1923.  Report  of  Dominion  Horticulturist,  p.  9. 

1924.  Report  of  Dominion  Horticulturist,  p.  10. 

Marshall,  Roy  E.,  Johnson,  H.  D.,  Hootman.  H.  D.,  and  Wells,  H.  M. 

1929.  Pollination  of  orchard  fruits  in  Michigan.  Mich.  Agr.  Exp.  Sta.  Spec.  Bui.  188. 
p.  38. 

Merrtll,  J.  H. 

1923.  Response  to  the  nectar  stimulus.     Amer.  Bee  Jour.  63:607. 

1924.  Bees   vs.    spraying.     Amer.   Bee   Jour.   64:134. 

MlNDERHOUD,     A. 

1931.  Untersuchungen  iiber  das  Betragen  der  Honigbiene  als  Bliitenbestauberin.  (Test 
on  the  behaviour  of  the  honev-bee  as  a  pollen  distributor.)  Gartenbauwissen- 
schaft.    4:342-62. 

MOFFETT,    A.    A. 

1931.     A   preliminary  account  of  chromosome  behaviour  in   the   Pomoideae.    Jour.  Pom. 
and  Hort.  Sci.  9:100-10. 
Morris.  O.  M. 

1921.     Studies   in  apple   pollination.     Wash.    Agr.   Exp.   Sta.   Bui.   163:1-32. 

MURNEEK,     A.    E. 

1930.  Horticultural  investigations  at   the  Missouri  Station.     Mo.  Agri.  Exp.  Sta.  Bui.  272. 
Ii930.     Horticulture  at  the   Missouri  Station.     Mo.  Agr.  Exp.  Sta.  Bui.  2C5. 

Nebel.  B. 

1930.  Recent  findings  in  cytology  of  fruits.  (Cvtologv  of  Pvrus  III).  Proc.  Amor. 
Soc.   Hort.   Sci.  27:406. 

OVERHOT.SER.     E.     L. 

1927.  Apple  pollination  studies  in  California.     Cal.   Agr.   Exp.  Sta.  Bui.  426:1-17. 
Park.   O.   W. 

1923.    Flight  studies  of  the  honey-bee.     Amer.  Bee  Jour.  63:71. 
Parker,  R.  L. 

1926.  The  collection  and  utilization  of  pollen  by  the  honevbee.  Cornell  Univ.  Agr. 
Exp.  Sta.   Mem.  98. 

Phillips,  E.  F. 

1927.  Some  effects  of  temperature  on  bee  activities.  Trans.  Iowa  St.  Hort.  Soc. 
no.   338-342. 

1928.  Bee-keeping. 

1930.     Honev-bees  for  the   orchard.     Corn.   Univ.   Agri.   Exp.   Sta.   Bui.    190:1-24. 
1930.     Bees  for  the  orchard.    Jour.  Econ.  Ent.  23:218-223. 


193 

Philp,   G.  S.   and  Vansell,   G.  H. 

1932.    Pollination    of    deciduous    fruits   by   bees.     Cal.    Agr.   Exp.   Sta.   Cir.  62. 
Price,  W.  A. 

1920.    Bees    and    their   relation    to    arsenical    sprays    at    blossoming    time.    Purdue    Agr. 
Exp.   Sta.   Bui.   247. 
Rawes,  A.  N.  and  Wilson,  G.  T. 

1922.    Pollen    carrying    agents.    Jour.    Roy.    Hort.    Soc.    47:15-17. 
Root,  A.  I.  and  Root,  E.  R. 

1929.    A  B  C  and  X  Y  Z  of  Bee-keeping. 
Rybin.  V.   A. 

1926.     Cytological  investigations  of  the  genus  Mains  (trans,  title).    Bui.  Appl.  Bot.  16. 

Sandsten,  E.  P. 

1909.     Some    conditions  which  influence   the   germination  and  fertility   of   pollen.    Univ. 
Wise.    Agr.   Exp.   Sta.   Res.   Bui.   4:149-172. 
Sax,  K. 

l!922.    Sterility    relationships    in    Maine    apple    varieties.     Maine    Agr.    Exp.    Sta.    Bui. 
307:61-76. 
Shoemaker,  J.  S. 

1926.    Pollen   development   in   the   apple.    Bot.   Gaz.   81:148-172. 
Simmons.  S. 

1904.     A  modern  bee  farm. 
Sutton,   Ida 

1918.    Report  on  tests  of  self-sterility  in  plums,  cherries,  and  apples  at  the  John  Innes 
Horticultural    Institution.    Jour.    Genet.    7:281-300;    Jour.    Pomol.    1:1-19. 
Tietz.  Harrtson   M. 

T924.    The  solubility  of  arsenate  of  lead  in  the  digestive  fluids  of  the  honey-bee.     (Apis 
mellifica).    Jour.   Econ.   Ent.    17:471. 
Vincent,   C.   C. 

1920.     Apple  pollination  studies  in  Idaho.     Better  Fruit.     14:11. 
Webster,  F.  M. 

1896.    Spraying  with   arsenites  vs.  bees.     Ohio   Agr.   Exp.   Sta.   Bui.   68:48-53. 

1894.    Spraying  with  arsenites  vs.  bees.     U.S.D.A.  Div.  Ent,  Insect  Life.     7:132. 
Weed,  A.  C. 

1918.    Value   of  bees  in  the   orchard.     Rural   N.Y.  77:831. 
Wellington,   R.,   Stout,   A.  B.,   Einset,   O.,   and  Van   Alstyne,   L.   M. 

1929.     Pollination    of    fruit    trees.     New    York    State    Agr.   Exp.    Sta.   Bui.   577:54. 
Wilson,  G.  F. 

1929.    Pollination    of    hardy    fruits;     insect    visitors    to    fruit    blossoms.    Annals    App. 
Biology.     15:602-629^ 
Woodrow,   A.  W. 

1932.    The  comparative  value  of  different  colonies  of  bees  in  pollination.    Jour.  Econ. 
Ent.  25:331-336. 


60796—13 


INDEX 


Acknowledgments,    12-13 

Andrena,  as  pollinators,  24,  92;  effect  of  tem- 
perature on  activity,   111;   constancy  of,   133 

Andrena  carlini  Ckll.,  as  pollinator,  94 

Andrena  ivilkella  Kirby,  as  pollinator,  94 

Anthomyids,  as  pollinators,  92 

Apis,  as  pollinators,  92;    constancy  of,   133 

Apis  mellifica  L.,  as  pollinator,  92 

Apple  industry  in  Nova  Scotia,   13-18 

Apple  scab,  see  Venturia  inaequalis  (Cook) 
Winter 

Apple  pollination,  experimental  studies  in, 
31-90 

Apple  varieties,  classification  of  (table),  8; 
genetic  constitution  of,  26-27;  chromosome 
numbers  of    (table),  26-27 

Arsenic  obtained  from,  bees  under  cage  con- 
ditions (table),  169;  larvae  (table),  169; 
summary  of  results  from  bees  (table),  170; 
pupae  (table),  170:  bees,  field  samples 
(table),  173;  pollen  (table),  180;  nectar 
(table),  180;  rain  water  (table),  182;  dan- 
delion and  strawberry  (table),  184;  pollen 
in  nest  of  wild  bees    (table),  186 

Arsenical  poisons,  effect  of.  on  bees,  164; 
effect  of,  on  bees  under  field  conditions,  175 

Auchter,  E.  C,  on  pollination  of  Cox  Orange, 
51;   pollination  of  King,  60 

Baldwin,  results  of  other  workers,  48;   selfing 
tests,    48;     as    female    parent,    48;     as    male 
parent,  48;    evidence  from  tented  series,  48, 
50;    summary   of    results   with    all    varieties, 
50;  general  summary,  50. 
Ballard,  W.  E.,  on  pollination  of   Stark,  78 
Barclay,  R.  A.,  on  bees  as  pollinators,  135 
Bees,    bumble,    discussion    of,    93;    species    list, 
94;    solitary,   species   list,   94;    discussion   of, 
94-96;    as    pollinators    under    controlled    con- 
ditions, previous  work  with,  31;  experiments 
with,    32-34;     influence    of    nectar    secretion 
and  availability  of  pollen  on  activity  of,  131- 
132 

Bee  activity,  at  different  diurnal  periods  re 
temperature  and  humidity  (table),  119;  in 
relation   to   nectar   secretion    (graph),   132 

Bee  colonies,  problems  in  distribution  and 
number,  140-156;  type  required  for  pollina- 
tion, 142-148;  observation  on  strength  of 
different  types,  144-148;  strength  of  over- 
wintered May  6/31  (table),  145;  strength 
of  over-wintered,  during  fruit  bloom,  146; 
types  of  experimental  studies  in,  146-148; 
method  of  placing,  154-155;  time  of  placing, 
155-156;    securing  of,   156-157 

Bee  counter,  photo-electric,  description  of, 
119-121 

Bee  poisoning,  list  of  common  poisons,  164; 
lethal  dosages,  165-166;  technique  of  chemi- 
cal tests  in,  166-167;  repulsion  tests,  171; 
feeding  tests,  171-172 


Bee,  hives,  discussion  of,  93;  utilization  of.  as 
orchard  pollinators,  134-157;  historical  re- 
view, 134-135;  experimental  use  of,  136- 
138;  when  to  be  used,  140;  flight  and 
concentration  of,  148-155;  effect  of  physical 
features  on  flight  and  concentration,  151; 
effect  of  weather  conditions  on  flight  and 
concentration,  151;  area  of  activity,  151; 
general  conclusions,   157 

Bees,  hive  and  wild,  relative  abundance  of. 
during  bloom,  108-109;  count  (table),  109; 
artificial  increase  and  distribution,  109; 
comparative  effect  of  climatic  factors  on, 
109-125;  effect  of  temperature  on,  111-113; 
effect  of  sunlight  and  solar  radiation  on 
activity  of.  113-117;  effect  of  wind  on  ac- 
tivity of,  117-118;  effect  of  humidity  on 
activity  of,  118;  effect  of  time  of  day  on 
activity  of,  118-119;  effect  of  climatic  factors 
on,  supplementary  data  of,  119-125;  number 
of  visits  of,  125;  pollen  gathering  habits  of. 
126-131;  comparative  constancy  of.  132-133; 
distribution  and  concentrations,  133;  gen- 
eral  summary,   133-134 

Blenheim,  experimental  results  of  other 
workers,  76;  of  selfing  tests,  76;  as  female 
parent,  76;  as  male  parent,  76:  summary  of 
all  varieties,  77;  general  summary  of,  78; 
fruit  set  of  tented  vs.  untented  trees,  78; 
effect   of  various  male  parents  on    (fig.),  81 

Blenheim,  Stark  cross,  fruitfulness  of  differ- 
ent types  of    (table),  81 

Bloom,  effect  of  condition  on  bee  activity. 
149;  varietal  atractiveness  of,  149;  effect  of 
masses  of,  150;  availability  of  other  bloom, 
150;  amount  available  in  relation  to  popula- 
tion present,   151-154 

Blooming  period,  of  different  varieties 
(charts),  83 

Blossoming  and  fruiting  habits  of  four  stand- 
ard varieties    (table),   30 

Bonnier,  G.,  on  area  of  activity  of  hive  bees, 

151 
Bochert,  A.,  bee  poisoning,   161 
Bouquets,  use  of  orchard  duster  with,  87-88 
Bourne,  A.  I.,  bee  poisoning,   160 
Bremus,  constancy  of,   133;    as  pollinators,  92 
Britton.  W.  E.  and  Viereck,  H.  L.,  on  insects 

as  pollinators,  91 
Bud-moth,  see  Tmeiocera  oceUana  Schiff. 
Burrell,    A.    B.    and    King,    G.    E.,    on    pollen- 

coater,  87 

Chironomidae,  as  pollinators,  93 
Chittenden,   F.   J.,   pollination   of  King,   60 
Climatic  factors,  comparative  effect  of,  on  hive 

and  wild  bees,   109-125 
Coke,  J.,  fertilizing  and  spraying,  18,  23 
Copper,  effect  of,  on  bees,  165 
Corrie,  L.  G.,  pollination  of  Cox  Orange,  51 
Cowan,  T.  W.,  bees  and  water,  181 


195 


196 


Cox  Orange,  results  of  other  workers.  51; 
selfing  tests.  51;  as  female  parent,  51;  as 
male  parent.  51;  evidence  from  tented 
series.  51;  summary  of  results  with  all 
varieties.  53;  general  summary,  53;  fruit- 
set  of  tented  vs.  untented  trees,  78 

Crane,  J.  E..  pollination  of  Cox  Orange,  51 

Crane.  M.  B.  and  Lawrence,  W.  J.  C,  un- 
fruitfulness,  24  and  26;  generational  ster- 
ility, 28;  incompatibility,  28;  pollination  of 
Cox  Orange,  51;  pollination  of  King,  60; 
pollination  of  Blenheim,  76 

Crosses,  with  standard  varieties  (table),  64; 
fruitfulness  of  different  types  of  (table). 
65;  Blenheim  and  Stark,  fruitfulness  of 
different  types  of    (table),  81 

Cross-fruitful,   definition,  24 

Cross-<pollination,  definition,  24;  conditions 
for,  31 

Cross-unfruitful,   definition,   24-25 

Cruciferae,  activity  of  hive  bees  on,   151 

Darlington,  C.  D.  and  Moffet,  A.  A.,  chromo- 
some behaviour  in  the  apple,  26 

Davis,  J.  J.,  on  bees  as  pollinators,  135 

DeOng.  E.  R.,  on  the  activity  of  honey  bees, 
110;  on  bees  as  pollinators,  135;  on  type  of 
colony  for  pollination,  143;  on  number  of 
colonies   per   acre   of   orchard,    152 

Detjen,    L.    R.,   on   morphological    sterility,   28 

Diptera,   on   apple  bloom,   99 

Doane,  R.  W.,  bee  poisoning,   159 

Dyce,  E.  J.,  number  of  blossoms  visited  In- 
bees,   125 

Effective  population,  definition  of,  100;  esti- 
mation of,  100,  102 

Einset,  O..  on  incompatibility.  28;  corre- 
lation between  seed  content  and  weight,  29; 
germination  of  pollen,  42 

EristaliSj  as  pollinator.  92 

Earrar,   C.  L.,   on   bee   trapping.   119;    on   type 

of    colony    for    pollination.    145;    on    method 

of  securing  colonies,  156 
Fertilization,  definition,  24 
Field   experiments,  with   hive  bees.   137-138 
Filmer,  R.  8.,  on  pollen  carriers.  128;   on  type 

of  colony  for  pollination,  144 
Flies   and  other    insects,   as   pollinators,   96.   99 
Forced    draft    pollination,    results    of    tests    in 

(table),  88 

Foreword,  11-12 

Fox-Wilson,  G.  F.,  on  insects  as  pollinators.  92 

Fruit  belt  in  Nova  Scotia,  map  of.  13:  geo- 
graphical position  of.  13-15;  climate  of,  15; 
apple  varieties  grown  in.  15-16;  quantity 
grown  in  (table),  17;  varietal  percentage  of 
total  crop  in,  17;   average  yields.   17-18 

Fruitful,  definition,  24 

Fruitfulness,  seed  content  in  relation  to.  65; 
of  different  types  of  Blenheim  and  Stark 
crosses    (table).    81 

Fruiting  habits  of  four  standard  varieties  for 
four  years,  30 

Fruit  production,  factors  other  than  pollina- 
tion affecting,  general.  18;  climatic  factors, 
19-22;  nutritional  factors,  21-22;  patho- 
logical factors,  22-23 

Fruit-set,  in  proportion  to  bloom.  29;  relation 
of,  on  tree  to  limb  or   spur,  86 


Fungicide  dust,  effect  of,  on  bees  under  field 
conditions,   173-174 

Gates,  B.  N\,  on  the  use  of  bees  as  pollinators, 
134 

Golden  Russet,  results  of  other  workers,  53; 
selfing  tests,  53;  as  female  parent,  55;  as 
male  parent,  55;  summary  of  results  with 
all   varieties.   55;    general  summary,   56 

Gowan.  J.  W.,  pollination  of  Northern  Spy,  63 

Gravenstein,  results  of  other  workers.  56; 
selfing  tests,  56;  as  female  parent,  56;  as 
male  parent,  56;  evidence  from  tented 
series,  58;  general  summary,  58;  summary 
of  results  with  all  varieties,  58 

Green  apple  bug,  see  Lygus  communis  Knight 

Halictus,  as  pollinators.  24,  92;   effect  of  tem- 
perature on  activity,  111;   constancy,  133 
Halictus  aicuatus  Robt..    as   pollinator,   94 
Halictus  craterus  Lov.,  as  pollinator,  94 
Halictus  lerouxii  Le  P.,  as  pollinator,  94 
Halictus  smilacinae  Robt.,  as  pollinator,  94 
Hand  pollination,  studies  in,  70-72;    as   an  al- 
ternative to   the  use  of  bouquets,  87 
Hedrick,     M.     P..     effect     of     temperature    on 

fruit-set.  19 
Hendriekson,  A.  X.,  on  bees  as  pollinators.  135 
Hilgendorff,   O..   and   Borchert.  A.,  bee   poison- 
ing.  160 
Hockey.   J.    F.    and    Harrison.   K.   A.,   carriage 

of  pollen  by  wind,  47 
Hooper.  C.  H.,  pollination  of  Cox  Orange.  51: 
insects  as  pollinators,  92:  number  of  colonies 
per   acre  of  orchard,   152 
Hewlett,     F.    S..    nutritional     factors.     21;     on 
g(  nerational    sterility.    28;    on    incompatibil- 
ity.  28;    Baldwin  pollination,  48 
Humidity,   effect  of.  on  bee  activity,  118 
Hutson,   K..  bees  as  pollinators,  31:   on   insecl 
pollinators.  93;    activity  of  honey  bees.   110; 
type    of    colony    for    pollination.    144:    flight 
and    concentration    of    bees,    148;    method   of 
placing  colonies,   154 

Incompatibility,  definition.  26;  discussion  of. 
28 

Insect  pollinators,  field  studies  in.  9  and  91- 
157:  introduction.  91;  general.  91.  100:  his- 
torical. 91-93;  relative  value  of.  100;  method 
of  study.  100 

Inter-fruitfulness.  studies  in.  7-8:  of  standard 
varieties,  studies  in.  37-74:  of  standard 
varieties,  summary,  69:  general  results  and 
conclusions  from  studies  in.  73;  chart  oi 
some   standard    varieties,   85 

King,  results  of  other  workers,  58,  60:   selfing 
tests,   60;     as   female    parent,   60;     as    male 
pa]  cut.  Oi):   evidence  fi  two   tented  sei  i<  - 
61;     summary    of    results    with    all    varieties, 
(il:    general  summary,  61 

Knowlton.  H.  E..  effect  of  temperature  on 
fruit-set,  19 

Kobel.  F.,  chromosome  behaviour  in  the  apple. 
26;  generational  sterility.  28;  false  par- 
thenocarpy,  29 

Lewis.  C.  1.  and  Vincent,  ('.  C,  pollination  of 

King.   60 
Langstroth,  L.  1..  bees  and  water.   181 
Lattimer,   L.   P.,  pollination  experiments,  -1 

Lygus   communis   Knight.   22 


197 


Lundie,  A.  E.,  on  the  activity  of  honey  bees, 
110,  112,  113:  on  bee  flight,  125;  bees  as 
pollinators,   136 

MacDaniels.  L.  H.,  on  flight  and  concentration 
of  bees,  148;  on  varietal  preference  of  bees, 
131;  on  activity  of  honey  bees,  111;  fruit- 
set,  29;  Baldwin  pollination,  48;  pollination 
of  Northern  Spy,  61;  on  over-pollination, 
86;  on  hand-pollination,  87 

MacDaniels.  L.  H.  and  Furr,  J.  R.,  destruction 
of  bloom  by  fungicides,  23 

MacDaniels.  L.  H.  and  Heinicke.  H.  A.,  on 
nutritional  factors,  21;  on  Baldwin  polli- 
nation, 48 

Macoun,  W.   T.,  bees   as  pollinators,   31 

Manglesdorf,  on  incompatibility,  28 

Marshall,  R.  E.,  et  al.,  on  method  of  securing 
colonies,   156 

Marshall,  R.  E.,  Johnson,  H.  D.  et  al.,  pollina- 
tion of  Northern  Spy,  61;  pollination  of 
Stark,  78;  on  activity  of  honey  bees,  110; 
on  methods  of  placing  colonies,   154 

Marshall,  R.  E.,  Johnson.  H.  D.,  Hootman, 
H.  D.  and  Wells,  H.  M.,  on  bees  as  polli- 
nators, 135-136 

McCulloch,  J.  W.,  on  number  of  blossoms 
visited  by  bees,  126 

Mclndoo,  H.  E.,  tent  experiments  in  bee 
poisoning,   166 

Mclndoo.  H.  E.  and  Demuth,  G.  H.,  on  bee 
poisoning,  158,  160;  on  repellent  action  of 
sprays  on  bees,   165 

Merrill,  J.  H.,  bee  poisoning,   159 

Minderhoud,  A.,  on  area  of  activity  of  hive 
bees,   151 

Moffet,  A.  A.,  chromosome  behaviour  in  the 
apple,  26 

Morphological  sterility,   28 

Morris,  O.  M..  bees  as  pollinators,  31;  Bald- 
win pollination,  48;  pollination  of  Gravenr 
stein,  56;  pollination  of  King,  60;  pollina- 
tion of  Stark,  78 

Mouldy  core,  occurrence  of,  66;  occurrence  and 
cause  of,  69 

Murneek,  A.  E.,  effect  of  weather  on  fruit- 
set,  19;  on  type  of  colony  for  pollination. 
144;  on  number  of  colonies  per  acre  of  or- 
chard,   152;    on   method   of   placing   colonies, 

Museidae,  as  pollinators,  93 

Nebel,  B.,  chromosome  behaviour  in  the  apple. 
26 

Nectar,  secretion,  and  availability  of  pollen, 
influence  on  bee  activity,  131-132;  secretion 
in  relation  to  bee  activity  (graph),  132; 
as  source  of  bee  poisoning,   180 

Ncpeta  hederacea  (L.)  Trevisan,  as  source  of 
bee  poisoning,  182 

Nicotine,  effect  of,  on  bees,  165;  effect  of,  on 
bees  under  field  conditions,  175 

Northern  Spy,  results  of  other  workers,  61, 
63;  selfing  tests,  63;  as  female  parent,  63; 
as  male  parent,  63;  evidence  from  tented 
senes,_  63-64;  summary  of  results  with  all 
varieties,   64;    general  summary,   64 

Nutritional  competition,  on  tented  trees 
effect  of    (table),  86 

Observation  stations,  102,  105 


Orchard     the    planning     of,     82-85;     suggested 

plan    for    five-variety     (fig.),    84;    suggested 

plan   for    four-variety    (fig.),    85 
Orchard    duster,    velocity   of    current   from     in 

relation  to   distance   from    (table).  89 
O'Smia  lignaria  Say,  as  pollinator,  92 
Overholser,  E.  L.,  pollination  of  Baldwin.  48; 

Gravenstein,  56;  King,  58 
Over-pollination,   86 

Park,  O.  W.,  on  activity  of  honey  bees,  111; 
on  bee  flight,  125 

Parker,  R.  L.,  on  pollen  gathering  habits  of 
bees,   126,   128 

Parthenocarpy,  definition,  26;  occurrence  of, 
29 

Phillips,  E.  F.,  the  activity  of  honey  bees, 
110;  bees  as  pollinators,  136;  type  of  colony 
for  pollination.  144;  method  of  securing 
colonies,   156;    bees  and  water,   181 

Philp,  G.  S.  and  Vansell,  G.  H.,  type  of  colony 
for  pollination,  143;  number  of  colonies  per 
acre  of  orchard,  152;  method  of  placing 
colonies,  154;  method  of  securing  colonies, 
157;  bee  poisoning,  161 

Poisoning  of  bees,  studies  in,  158-189;  intro- 
duction, 158;  historical.  158-161;  develop- 
ment of,  161-163;  experiments  in,  163-166; 
tent  experiments  in,  166-172;  studies  from 
commercial  orchards.  172-178;  influence  of 
weather  on,  178-179;  sources  of  (food),  180- 
182;  sources  of  (plants),  182,  184;  influence 
of  size  of  colony  and  of  apiary  on,  186;  in- 
fluence of  time  of  placing  in  orchard  on,  186; 
evidence  of,  amongst  wild  bees,  186,  188; 
summary  of  poison  tests  on,   188-189 

Pollen,  germination  of  (table),  41-42;  germ- 
ination, optimum  temperature  for,  44;  inter- 
fruitf ulness  of,  69;  different  varieties,  diag. 
showing  value  of,  on  standard  varieties,  70; 
different  varieties,  graphs  showing  value  of, 
on  Spy,  Blenheim,  Stark,  and  Gravenstein, 
71;  on  Golden  Russet,  King,  Baldwin,  and 
Cox  Orange,  72;  temporary  provision  of, 
87;  unfruitful,  inhibiting  effect  of,  89-90; 
as  source  of   bee  poisoning,   180 

Pollenrcoater,  87 

Pollen-gathering,  habits  of  hive  and  wild  bees, 

126-131 
Pollen  supplies,  source  of,  156 
Pollen  tests,  method,  40-41;   results,  41-47 
Pollination,   and   fertilization,   process   of,   5-6; 
definition  of,  24-25;   problem  of,  6-7  and  25; 
results  of  experiments,  47;   tests  with  Blen- 
heim  and   Stark,   74-82;    time   necessary  for, 
140-142;     effect    of    length     of     exposure    on 
(table),    141;     influence     of     length    of    ex- 
posure on    (table),   142 

Pollinator,   definition,  24 

Pollinizers.  arrangement  of,  9-10;  definition  of, 
24 

Pollinizing  bouquets,  87 

Price,  W.  A.,  bee  poisoning,  159 

Raphanus  Baphanistrum  L..  as  source  of  bee 
poisoning,   182 

Rawes,    A.    N.    and   Wilson,   G.    T.,   on   insects 

as   pollinators,   92 
Root,  A.  I.  and  Root,  E.  R.,  bee  keeping,  181 

Roscoe,  M.  V.,  acknowledgment  unpublished 
data,  12;  chromosome  behaviour  in  the 
apple,  26 


198 


Ryihin,  V.  A.,  chromosome  behaviour  in  the 
'apple,   26 

Sandsten.  E.  P.,  relation  of  nutrition  to  qual- 
ify of  pollen,  22 

Sax,  K.,  pollination  of  Golden  Russet,  53;  on 
bees  as  pollinators,   135 

Scarabaeidae,   as  pollinators,  93 

Seed  content,  relation  to  fruit-fulness,  65;  re- 
lation to  weight,  65;  morphological  abnor- 
malties    associated   with,    68-69 

Self-fertile,   definition,  25 

Self-fruitful,   definition,   24 

Self-fruitf ulness,  seasonal  variation  in,  90; 
due  to   technique,  90 

Self-pollination,   definition,   24 

Self-sterile,   definition,  25 

Self-unfruitful,   definition,  24 

Simmins,  S..  on  pollen  and  nectar  gathering 
habits  of  bees,  127 

Solar  radiation  apparatus,  relative  response 
of  cell  and  filters  of,  113 

Sprays,  repellent  action  on  bees,  165 

Stark,  experimental  results,  of  other  workers, 
78;  selling  tests,  78;  as  female  parent,  78, 
80;  as  male  parent,  80;  all  varieties,  sum- 
mary of,  80;  general  summary  of,  80;  effect 
of  various  male  parents  on   (fig.),  81 

Stark-Blenheim  cross,  fruitfulness  of  different 
types  of    (table),  81 

Sterility,  generational,  28;  morphological, 
definition.  26;  definition,  25;  generational, 
definition,  26 

Sulphur,  effect  of,  on  bees,  165 

Sulphur  dust,  effect  of,  on  bees  under  field 
conditions,    174-175 

Summary,   popular,   5-10 


Sunlight  and  solar  radiation,  effect  on  activ- 
ity of  hive  and  wild  bees,  113-117 

Sutton,    I.,    pollination    of    Cox   Orange,    51 

Syrphus,  as  pollinators.,  92 

Taraxacum  officinale  Weber,  activity  of  bees 
on,  151 

Temperature,  effect  on  hive  and  wild  bees, 
111-113;  relation  of  hive  bee  activity  in 
apple  bloom   to    (table),   111. 

Tent  experiments,  general  results  of,  34-36; 
humidity  and  temperature  records  of,  35; 
with  hive  bees,  136-137;  results  of  (table), 
136 

Tent  studies,  summary  of  (table),  36;  in  in- 
terfruitfulness,   69 

Tietz,  H.  M.,  bee  poisoning,  159 

Time  of  day,  influenced  of,  on  bee  activity, 
118-119 

Tmetocera  ocellana  Schiff.,  22 

Top-working,   plan  for   Blenheim    (fig.),   84 

Trachandrena,  as  pollinators,  92 

Trifolium,   activity   of  hive  bees  on,   151 

Unfruitf ulness,  other  causes  of,  10;  causes  of, 
26 

Vincent,  C.   C,  pollination  of   Gravenstein.   56 
Venturia  inaeqnalis    (Cook)    Winter,  22 

Water,   as   source  of   bee   poisoning,   181-182 
Webster,  F.  M.,  bee  poisoning,  158 
Weed,  A.  C,  on  bees  as  pollinators,  134-135 
Wellington,  Stout  et  al.,  pollination  of  Graven- 
stein,  56;    pollination  of   King,   60 
Wind,   effect   on  bee   activity.   117-118 
Wind    velocity,    inside    and    outside    tent,    com- 
parison   of    (table),    89 
Woodrow,  A.  W..  on  honey  bee   activity.   Ill; 
on   types  of   colony   for   pollination,    14.1 


OTTAWA 

J.   O.   PATENAUDE,   ACTING   KING'S   PRINTER 

1933 


*•  P  I